OVERDUE FINES: 25¢ per W per item RETUMIMB LIBRARY MTERIALS: ___________.__——— Place in book return to muove dune frat circuhtion records 'o-n. I ’ 3““ L- x (sir-m“ V‘ ‘ ~ILQ‘uul V x “ "” 4 PRIMARY RELATIONS AND SOCIAL ORGANIZATION: A THEORETICAL AND EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION BY Subhash Durlabhji A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfilmment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Management 1981 mugs: tc 335.11.28.11} Zaintenanc View as that 15' ? SOCiety. ac“mam {Galatians logical w! Work traditiOng as a SOCiEI allowing 1 Problems 3 ! CE-II5‘“7J“C: ABSTRACT PRIMARY RELATIONS AND SOCIAL ORGANIZATION: A THEORETICAL AND EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION BY Subhash Durlabhji Views of scholars from the fields of organization behavior, psychology, and sociology on the question of the determinants of individual and societal well-being are brought together in this dissertation. The role of work organizations in the enhancement of individual and societal well-being is explored. A set of propositions are developed from this inter- disciplinary survey. Primary relations, that is, relations constituted as ends in themselves rather than as means to an end, are viewed as being necessary for the achievement and maintenance of psychological equilibrium. They are also viewed as being necessary for achievement of moral solidarity, that is, a degree of homogeneity of values of members of society. It is suggested that industrialization is accompanied by the erosion of traditional sources of primary relations, resulting in a loss of psychological and socio- logical well-being. Work organizations are proposed as an alternative to traditional sources of primary relations. Japan is presented as a society in which work organizations do play this role, allowing Japan to avoid many of the social and psychological problems that often accompany industrialization. Aspects of cozpanie project: measures well-bei relation- individu at the 0; Japanese work organization considered encouraging of primariness of the individual's relationship with the organization (PIRO) which may be applicable to the American context are combined to yield the notion of TYPE of organization. An exploratory investigation with eight manufacturing companies was conducted, using interviews, and semi- projective (sentence-completion) and objective survey measures. Relationships among PIRO, TYPE, and the emotional well—being (Egg) of respondents were examined. A strong relationship between £159 and EEE was obtained, at the individual level of analysis (r = .36, s = .002, N = 64) and at the organizational level of analysis (r = .72, s = .02, N = 8). Relationships between TXPE and 3339 (r = .30, n.s., N = 8) and between TXPE and EEE (r = .33, n.s., N = 8) were not significant, but in the expected direction. I . iisserta 98:26:35: access;t ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am extremely fortunate to have been guided in this dissertation by a committee of professors whose patience and generosity went well beyond the call of duty. Their ready accessibility, their careful review of my efforts, and, most especially, their abundance of ideas in response to my needs made this dissertation an enriching experience for me. I wish I had been able to incorporate all of their valuable suggestions in the work reported here. Flaws in the dissertation should be attributed to an inadequate graSp of their suggestions by me, and not at all to any lack of attention on their part. It is impossible to separately specify the contributions made by each member of the committee. Dr. Stan Stark continued to guide, and not impose, as dissertation committee chairman, as he has done since 1975 as my adviser. His influence on the ideas contained in this dissertation are fundamental. I am also grateful to him for rating inter- view data. My association with Dr. Ken White has been almost as long; his contributions to my development have not been any less. Dr. Ben Schneider insisted on my learning a few basic lessons; I am extremely grateful that he did. I called upon Dr. Harold Teitlebaum, who was not formally on ii I' Q n-vgnon “O. b- v- o 12 ZCST Va 51. ~ in; hue .4 fcb wit": 0’ . nah-Jon‘- - ‘.“V“Lh - as well. the committee, almost as much as I called upon the committee members; his help was invaluable. Many other people at this university have contributed, directly or indirectly, to the ideas presented here. Among the most prominent of these are: Dr. Eugene Jennings, Dr. Winston Oberg, Dr. Henry Tosi, and Dr. John Wanous in Management; Dr. John McKinney and Dr. Ellen Strommen, in Psychology; Dr. Chris Vanderpool and Dr. John Gullahorn, in Sociology; and Dr. Warren Samuels, in Economics. As raters for the sentence-completion instruments used in the dissertation I needed sharp people who would do the job with dedication. In Dave Bowen, Linda Kohl, and Dave VanHouten I found the perfect raters--and perceptive critics as well. Ginger Noell typed two or three drafts and the final dissertation with astonishing speed and accuracy. As the fourth rater and as my wife, Neelam Kher has been a constant source of support and encouragement. The peace and stability that she brings to my life enhances every facet of my existence and allows me to devote all my energies to my work. iii ?\'--y~ u... i -Joo-Vu» ”L. “to: r-c .U n (D '1 T? “. TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 0 PART I: LITERATURE SYNTHESIS Chapter I. PRIMARY RELATIONS AS A DETERMINANT MENTAL HEALTH . . . . . . . . . . Primary Relations . . . . . . . Psychological Equilibrium . a . Anomie . . . . . . . . . . . Perceived Efficacy . . . . . OF Relationship between Primary Relations and Psychological Equilibrium . Role of Primary Group Membership Achievement of Psychological Equilibrium: Anomie . . . . Role of Primary Group Membership Achievement of Psychological Equilibrium: Alienation . . Maintenance of Psychological Equilibrium . . . . . . . . Durkheim . . . . . . . . Marx . . . . . . . . . . Homans . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . II. PRIMARY RELATIONS AS A DETERMINANT SOCIETAL HEALTH 0 O O O I O C O O Societal Health and "Moral Solidarity". Societal Health . . . . . . "Moral Solidarity . . . . . "Moral Solidarity" and Primary Relations . . . . . . . . . . Durkheim . . . . . . . . . . Cooley . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . III. THE GREAT TRANSFORMATION: THE ATTENUATING EFFECT OF MODERNIZATION ON PRIMARY RELATIONS . O O C C I O C O O O 0 iv 12 13 15 19 20 23 25 25 27 28 28 30 30 30 35 38 38 43 44 47 as I)” votub . (I Wt hu- ‘0 ' \ Chapter - _ Tonnies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Weber . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 Stinchcombe . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 Warren . . . . . . . . . . . 55 Cooley, Wirth,Ange11, Kasandra and Janowitz . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 IV. ORGANIZATIONAL PRIMARY RELATIONS AS A DETERMINANT OF SOCIETAL, ORGANIZATIONAL, AND EMPLOYEE HEALTH . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 The Potential of Work Groups . . . . . . . 67 China . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 Japan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 Drawbacks of the Chinese and Japanese ‘ Organizational Forms . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Type "A" and Type "Z" . . . . . . . . . . . 79 Outcomes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 PART II: EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION V. HYPOTHESES AND OPERATIONAL DEFINITIONS . . . . 88 Hypotheses of the Empirical 88 Investigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Definitions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 TYPE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 Emotional Well- -Being (EWB) . . . . . . . 103 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 VI. SAMPLE AND DATA COLLECTION . . . . . . . . . . 114 Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114 Data Generation Procedures . . . . . . . . 118 Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization . . . 119 TYPE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 Emotional Well-Being . . . . . . . . . . 122 Demographic Information . . . . . . . . 127 Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 VII. DATA ANALYSIS RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . 129 Missing Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 Internal Consistency . . . . . . . . . . . 130 V 1.--? i.-. O Chapter Multi-Trait Multi-Method Matrix . . Organizational Level Analysis . Results . . . Discussion . Summary of Results Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis thH-0 VIII. IMPLICATIONS AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . Limitations on Interpretation . . . Construct Validity . . . . . . . . APPENDIX A . APPENDIX B . APPENDIX C . APPENDIX D . Generalizability . . . . . . . . Interpretations of Correlations Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) . . . . . . . . . . . . TYPE . . . . . . . . . . . Emotional Well-Being . Conclusions . . . . . LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . ._. . vi 133 136 140 146 146 150 152 154 156 157 158 158 160 163 168 169 172 176 179 183 193 199 201 207 4- O C), o \I o 10. ll. Table 1. 10. 11. 9A LIST OF TABLES Main Distinctions between Primary and Secondary Realtions . . . . . . . . . . . Two Ideal Types of Organizational Control Descriptive Information about Sample companies 0 O O O O O O O O O O O C I O O Scales Used to Measure Variables . . . . Internal Consistency Coefficients - Standardized Item Alpha (N = 64) . . . . Reliability Coefficients, Combined Ratings, Sentence Completion Scales (N = 64) . . . Multi-Trait Multi-Method Matrix (N = 64) Correlation Matrix, "TYPE" Scales (N = 8) Correlation Coefficients, PIRO, TYPE, and EWB (N = 8) O O C O I O O O C O O O 0 Correlation Coefficients, PIRO, TES, and EWB (N = 8) o I o o o o o o o o o o 0 Correlation Coefficients, Two Indices of Employment Stability with PIRO and EWB Correlation Coefficients Corrected for Attenuation, PIRO, TYPE and EWB (N = 8) . vii Page 11 81 115 126 131 132 134 138 141 143 144 145 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. The Activation Continuum . . . . . . . . . . 108 2. Measurement Structure . . . . . . . . . . . 139 viii INTRODUCTION The nature of interpersonal relations predominant in a society is viewed in this study as having a profound influence on individual and societal well-being. Consequences of changes in the nature of interpersonal relations brought about by industrialization and modernization are examined. The possible role of work organizations in alleviating some of the more dysfunctional of these consequences is suggested. This dissertation is divided into two parts. Part I consists of a theoretical investigation of primary relations and social organization. A guiding conviction in this discussion is that psychological, organizational, and sociological phenomena cannot be studied in isolation from each other. An interdisciplinary review and synthesis of the literature results in the formulation of the following propositions: 1. Individuals need primary relations to achieve and maintain psychological equilibrium. 2. The stability and adaptability of a society depend on its ability to develop "moral solidarity" in its members. 3. In traditional societies, the system of primary relations was the mechanism 1 2 through which such solidarity was developed. 4. As societies modernize and industrialize, the traditional sources of primary relations weaken. 5. Work organizations, through their choice of control mechanisms, are able to provide the environment in which primary relations can develop. The outcomes would relate not only to the emotional equilibrium of individuals and the moral solidarity or integration of society as a whole, but also to the efficiency and effectiveness of the organization itself. These propositions are the subject of discussion in Part I (Chapters I through IV). The objectives of this discussion are limited to elaboration of the concepts contained in the propositions and of the meaning of the propositions. Arguments made by various writers that serve to support the propositions are presented. These arguments are rarely supported by "data," in the current sense of the word. While this does not mean they are not empirically derived, their appeal is mostly to logic. No critique of the methodology used by these writers is presented in this dissertation, though inclusion here implies its evaluation by the writer. t0 c01* | The 8a. in thit 3 The issues raised earlier about the sources of individual and societal well-being have been addressed by so many scholars in so many fields that it is beyond the capacity of a single individual to review, let alone synthesize, all that has been written on the subject. The selectivity imposed upon the available material is influenced, in the final analysis, by the writer's values and biases. These propositions then form the point of departure for Part II of the dissertation, which is a report of empirical testing of a small subset of the assertions contained in the propositions. The work of Ouchi and his colleagues (Johnson and Ouchi, 1974; Ouchi, 1981; Ouchi and Johnson, 1978; Ouchi and Price, 1978) provided the impetus for the effort reported here, which may be viewed as a replication and extension of their work. ' Chapter V translates prOposition 1 and part of proposition 5 into testable hypotheses. The variables that emerge from the effort in Part I are described here in much more detail, especially with the objective of constructing instruments capable of measuring them. Chapter VI describes the instruments which were used to collect data; some of these instruments were developed for this study, while others are standardized measures. The sample from which data were obtained is also described in this chapter. 4 Chapter VII describes the analysis performed on the data. The results‘of‘the analysis and hypotheses-testing are discussed, and some tentative conclusions are reached. The exploratory nature of the empirical investigation is emphasized. Chapter VIII contains a discussion of the lessons learned from this research experience, pertaining to the research process itself and to the conceptual or theoretical aspects. Directions for future research are indicated. The most important of these are summarized in the last section of the chapter, along with the major conclusions of the research. PART I LITERATURE SYNTHESIS CHAPTER I PRIMARY RELATIONS AS A DETERMINANT OF INDIVIDUAL MENTAL HEALTH This chapter elaborates upon PrOposition l, which states that individual mental health cannot be achieved without a history of primary group membership, nor can it be maintained without continued membership in the same or different primary groups. In the absence of a background of being related to others in primary relationships, individuals will grow up to be alienated and anomic, anxious and hostile. And when an otherwise psychologically balanced individual loses his/her primary relationships,‘ s/he will be unable to maintain his/her psychological balance for long without finding other primary relationships. The concept of "primary relations" is described first, then some possible components of "psychological equilibrium." Finally, the relationship between these two concepts, as stated in Proposition 1, is discussed. Primary Relations Primary relations are those relations among individuals typically found in primary groups. The term "primary group" comes to us from Cooley (1962). "By primary groups I mean 7 those characterized by intimate face-to-face association and cooperation" (page 23). As examples of the most important primary groups, Cooley lists the family, the play group of children, and the neighborhood or community group of elders. Cooley wrote in 1909, hence it is not certain that he would characterize the modern-day counterparts of these groups as primary. Even then Cooley noted that "the intimacy of the neighborhood has been broken up by the growth of an intricate mesh of wider contacts which leaves us strangers to peOple who live in the same house" (page 26). Ritchie and Koller (1964) have drawn up a more extensive list of characteristics that distinguish primary groups from what we may call secondary groups. Primary groups tend to involve the whole personalities of their members, rather than fragments or segments of personalities as in secondary groups. They demand and achieve face-to- face relationships over long periods of time, whereas secondary groups rely more heavily upon intervening media such as pictures, letters, advertisements, and formal forms of discourse utilized more intermittently. Primary groups are characterized by a Spontaneous, unrehearsed, relaxed informality, in contrast with the structured, formal procedures common in secondary groups. Wirth (1938), in describing urban life as consisting of mostly secondary rather than primary contacts among people, suggests that, in secondary contacts, the persons tend to 8 stand in a relationship of utility to each other in the sense that the role which they play in each other's life is overwhelmingly regarded as a means for the achievement of their own ends. This distinction between relationships as a means to an end and relationships as ends in themselves forms the basis for Tonnies (1940) famous theory of Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft. Tonnies seeks to suggest in this treatise that societies exhibit a decided historical change from being predominantly Gemeinschaft to being predominantly Gesellschaft. These are distinguished from each other primarily by the form of relationships among people predominant in each type. The fundamental difference in the form of relationship relates to the kind of human will that underlies the relationship. When a group or a relationship is willed because those involved wish to attain through it a definite end and are willing to join hands for this purpose, even though indifference or even antipathy may exist on other levels, then the basis for the relationship is rational will, in which means and ends have been sharply differentiated. If people associate because they think the relationship valuable as an end in and of itself, it is natural will that predominates. Tonnies identifies five main Gemeinschaft ties, characterized by being based predominantly on natural will: between mother and child, father and child, sisters and brothers, friend and Spontan is mad Expres 9 friend, and rulers and subjects. By contrast, of course, Gesellschaft ties are the kind that exist between business- men and employees, professional and client, and so on. Warren (1972), in his discussion of the "great change" in American communities that we will refer to in Chapter III, specifies the following distinction between primary and secondary groups: Where the primary group is intimate, involving the participation of the 'whole' personality, the secondary group is more casual, and individuals participate with only that segment of their personality which represents the shared interest . . . a rigid dichotomy is not indicated, but rather a continuum between these two extremes (page 60). The last sentence is significant; primary and secondary are ideal types that, to a certain extent, coexist in the same relationship. Thus, while groups in work organizations are predominantly secondary, they are not devoid of primary group characteristics. The reCOgnition of the distinction between "task functions" and "maintenance functions" in Organization Behavior attests to this. What Thibaut and Kelly (1959) wish to point out in Specifying this distinction is that, in addition to activities directly serving the ends for which the group has been constituted, members engage in activities and interactions which serve an integrative function, and which are based more on Spontaneous relationships based on sentiment than on the formal structure of the group. A parallel distinction is made by Parsons between Instrumental Activity and Expressive Activity, and is mentioned in this context 10 by Warren (1972). It is this same distinction that forms the basis for the distinction between "Initiating Structure" and "Consideration" roles of the leader, or, as Kerr and Jermier (1978) put it, for the recognition that people in work groups seek "guidance" as well as "good feelings." The description of primary relations may be rounded out by reference to the ideas of philosopher-poet Buber (1952), who suggests that people tend to look at other people and objects either as a series of "Its" and relate themselves to each It in an I-It relationship, or alternatively, as "Thous" and relate themselves to each Thou in an I-Thou relationship. For Buber, the I-Thou relationship is distinguished from the I-It as mutual sympathy and compassion are distinguished from manipulation and exploitation. One might wish for more neutral descriptors than "manipulation" and "exploitation" for relationships in which rational will predominates; in any case, it is clear that secondary relations may be expected to have more of the I-It element than primary relations. Table 1 summarizes the main distinctions between primary and secondary relations. This discussion of primary relations may be closed with some remarks of Maslow's (1954) which are concerned with his understanding of self-actualizing persons. His description of the "love of a self-actualizing person" is remarkably similar to what we have called a primary relationship. 11 There is too much talk in the psychological literature of rewards and purposes, of reinforcements and gratifications, and not nearly enough of what we may call the end experience (as contrasted with the means experience) . . . Horney in a lecture has defined unneurotic love in terms of regarding others as per se, as ends in themselves, rather than as” means 32 ends (pages 254-255; emphaSIs added). TABLE 1 MAIN DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN PRIMARY AND SECONDARY RELATIONS Primary Secondary Based on Natural Will Based on Rational Will Ends in themselves Means to an end Whole persons involved Segments of persons involved Face-to-face interaction Mediated interaction Long-term Often brief Frequent contact Intermittent contact Spontaneous and informal Structured and formal Warm, intimate, personal Indifferent and impersonal I-Thou I-It In this section, one of the key concepts of Proposition 1 has been described in some detail. In the next section, the other key concept of PrOposition 1, psychological equilibrium, is described. It may be noted at the outset that, for the purposes of this project, the terms "mental health," "emotional well-being," and "psychological equilibrium" are considered to be equivalent. 0 seek s as the Oke'; ‘ cze 2'. pee; e ng‘ on «o '1 (I: (D U) '1 t. I. (h m n own 0 Oh“) () OI : mwuzoo W add t‘-, Confid. 12 Psychological Equilibrium Individuals interact with their environments as they seek satisfaction of their animal and human needs, and as they seek to exercise their capabilities. In doing so, they face many Situations containing potential problems some minor, some major, many of them involving other people. Bradburn (1969) arrives at a definition of "mental health" in the following passage. The fact that human beings are not self- sufficient but must live in an interdependent society where other human beings are also engaged in the pursuit of their goals and desires leads inevitably to a succession of interpersonal conflicts. For the most part, the rules of social life establish a framework in which individuals are able to interact with each other and to help each other toward the mutual satisfaction of their goals. But inevitably situations occur in which the course of life does not go smoothly, and problems arise. The ability to c0pe with these difficult situations without undue pain to oneself or others is one of the common criteria used for distinguishing 'mental health' (page 2). White (1959) develops the concept of "competence" as "an organism's capacity to interact effectively with its environment." While this is a more neutral concept than Bradburn's definition of mental health in that it makes no reference to avoiding "undue pain to others," one may reasonably assume that "effective" interaction at least subsumes "without undue pain to others." Without disagreeing with Bradburn or White, one may add that this ability to c0pe affords a measure of self- confidence to the mentally healthy individual which 13 characterizes his or her dealings with the environment. It is the facing of each day and each situation without anxiety that defines for us an individual who has achieved psycholOgical equilibrium; and if the individual is able to emerge from each situation without any substantial loss of equilibrium, then he has succeeded in maintaining his psychological equilibrium. This definition applies equally to the socially adapted individual and to the individual engaged in socially maladapted behavior: a criminal is 'not automatically to be characterized as lacking in mental health. Ouchi and Johnson's (1978) pioneering work, which, as stated before, this study will attempt to partially replicate and extend, measures "emotional well-being" by the following dimensions: happiness, doing well in life, positive affect, perceived efficacy, anxiety, hostility, negative affect, and anomie. In the following pages two of Ouchi and Johnson's eight dimensions--Anomie and Perceived Efficacy--are discussed in some detail, because of their special relevance to the present theoretical framework. Anomie Anomie is most commonly conceptualized as "normlessness . " The scale used by Ouchi and Johnson (1978) measures an individuals "generalized pervasive sense of self-to-others belongingness at one extreme compared with self-to-other distance and self-to-others alienation at the other pole of the continuum" (pages 305-306). With reference to the C 3. C I 210:3 14 passage from Bradburn (1969) quoted above, Anomie is the subjective state of individuals who do not have in their possession the "framework of rules of social life." Durkheim (1951), who brought the concept into the mainstream of sociological thought, thinks of Anomie as the lack of "moral regulation" in the activities of individuals. Gould and Kolb (1965) have abstracted from the work of MacIver the following description of Anomie: The fulfillment of the process of desocial- ization, the retreat of the individual into his own ego, the Skeptical rejection of all social bonds . . . the state of mind of one who has been pulled up from his moral roots, who has no longer any standards but only disconnected urges, who has no longer any sense of continuity, of folk, of obligation (page 9). Blauner (1964) thinks of Anomie as "social alienation," and other writers have given slightly different interpretations of the concept, as a perusal of the collection of readings edited by Marcson (1970) makes evident. There is ample justification for viewing Anomie as a component of poor mental health, since it is clear that Anomie is likely to be an uncomfortable state in and of itself. Thus, Durkheim (1951) traces the cause of a peculiarly modern type of suicide to Anomie. It is also likely that the anomic individual's dealings with the environment will be wrought with anxiety as he attempts to produce behaviors that would result in satisfying outcomes without the help of "rules of social life." One way to conceptualize this Y“ " eure. EXCEI 5.;- V V Coo ‘ O to! .. "ufi‘ . v -c- .E.;- ssvoc h) V-» a-# Haey of wt 15 experience would be to suggest that the anomic individual experiences each Situation as unique and unrelated to any other situation. This is equivalent to being confronted with more stimulation than the individual is able to relate to. Such overstimulation would lead to anxiety, stress, and immobility. Another image useful in this context is to view social norms as the rudder with the help of which individuals steer through the innumerable social situations of daily living. The lack of direction, control, and stability suggested by the image of a rudderless ship in often turbulent waters is an apt representation of the experience of the anomic individual. Anomie is an important concept in this dissertation for another reason, namely, as the psychological counterpart of the idea of "moral solidarity," which is a key concept in Propositions 2 and 3. While "moral solidarity" will be discussed in detail in Chapter II, it might briefly be suggested here that a society lacking "moral solidarity" is a society composed of anomic individuals. Perceived Efficacy Inkeles and Smith (1974), the source from which Ouchi and Johnson (1978) obtained their scale for Perceived Efficacy, define the concept in terms of the belief that man can learn how to exert considerable control over his environment. The efficacious individual would tend to advance his own goals, rather than being dominated by the forces created by more powerful men or by nature itself. '11 u-.‘ofl . 16 The sense of efficacy . . . includes, as well, the sense that one can effectively do something if officials are proposing what one considers to be a bad law, and the belief that care will help prevent accidents, that human nature can be changed, that men can arrange their affairs so that even nations can live in peace. [The efficacious individul] would express his confidence in his ability, alone and in concert with other men, to organize his life so as to master the challenges it presents at the personal, the interpersonal, the communal, the national, and even the international levels (Inkeles and Smith, 1974, page 22). While Ouchi and Johnson view Perceived Efficacy as a component of emotional well-being, it may be argued that Perceived Efficacy is by itself simply an orientation or generalized expectancy acquired by the individual from his experiences in a particular cultural milieu. Lack of Perceived Efficacy may or may not be associated with poor mental health. For example, the perception of Personal Efficacy in the face of uncontrollable negative events may result in feelings of guilt and in disequilibrium. The relationship of Perceived Efficacy to psychological equilibrium is evident, however, in the contribution it may make to the self-confidence of the individual, a characteristic of mental health suggested earlier. White (1959) also talks about a "feeling of efficacy" in the context of his discussion on competence. One may also note the striking similarity between the concept of Perceived Efficacy and Internal Locus of Control, a concept developed by Rotter (1966) that has subsequently become a central construct of personality theory (see Lefcourt, 1972, 1976; ...... LJ r3 to one result Stance: Scale ¢ Percei‘ 0f man I'() (j :1. p. ._ 17 Phares, 1976). Lefcourt (1972), in a major review article, 'defines Locus of Control as: The degree to which individuals perceive the events in their lives as being a consequence of their own actions, and thereby controllable (internal control), or as being unrelated to their own behaviors and, therefore, beyond personal control (external control). It is a generalized expectancy, an abstraction developed from a host of experiences in which expectancies have been met with varying degrees of validation (page 2). Throughout the book written four years later, Lefcourt (1976) refers to this concept as "perceived control," bringing it even closer to the idea of Perceived Efficacy being discussed here as a possible contributor to confidence in one's ability to c0pe with contingencies presented by the environment. The perception that events in one's life are not related to one's actions and are therefore uncontrollable may also result in feelings of powerlessness under certain circum- stances. Lefcourt and Ladwig (1965) used a Powerlessness scale developed by Dean (1969) as a "second" test of perceived control. Powerlessness is a fundamental characteristic of Alienation. Marx, whose characterization of man in capitalist society as alienated remains the starting point for all discussions of the phenomenon, finds the root of Alienation in the relationship of labor to its product. . . . the object produced by labor, its product, now stands opposed to it as an alien being, as a power independent of the producer. The alienation o the worker in his product means not only that his his labor becomes an object, assumes an external N! Ln! (1 m 18 existence, but that it exists independently outside himself, and alien to him, and that it stands opposed to him as an autonomous power (Marx, 1963; pages 122-123). For Marx, the "product of labor" that now stands opposed to the worker as an autonomous power is capital, the means of production. Marx finds the first characteristic 1 finds itself in a of alienation in.the fact that labor relationship of relative powerlessness to its own product, the means of production. Ollman (1971), in his analysis of Alienation, also attaches primary importance to the relation- ship between Alienation anui Powerlessness: ". . . Marx claims that one of the manifestations of alienation is that 'all is under the sway of inhuman power'" (page 132). Powerlessness as an aspect of Alienation is a fundamental precept of more recent students of Alienation as well. Faunce (1968) describes Alienation as Powerlessness, Normlessness and Meaninglessness. Melvin Seeman (1959) adds 1It is important to remember that by "labor" Marx refers not to workers but activity of workers; furthermore for Marx "worker" is not the "blue-collar worker" on whom modern studies of alienation have primarily focused (see Marcson, 1970), but all individuals who work--that is, mankind as a whole. The fact that it is not merely the proletariat but "man in capitalist society" who is alienated is brought forth clearly in Ollman's (1971) analysis of Alienation: "The connection Marx sees between proletarian alienation and that of the rest of mankind is expressed in his claim that 'the whole of human servitude is involved in the relation of the worker to production, and every relation of servitude is but a modification and consequence of this relation.'" Not only "workers" but capitalists and consumers as well suffer from Alienation. Alienated work has the character that "it is not the satisfaction of a need, but only a means for satisfying other needs" (Marx, 1964; page 125). Capitalists and managers are not excluded from this phenomenon. 19 to this list two further meanings of Alienation-~Isolation and Self-Estrangement. In his discussion of Alienation as Powerlessness, he writes: The idea of alienation as powerlessness is, perhaps, the most frequent usage in current literature. The contributors to Alvin Gouldner's volume on leadership, for example, make heavy use of this idea; as does the work of C. Wright Mill--and, I suppose, any analysis of the human condition that takes the Marxist tradition with any seriousness. This variant of alienation can be conceived as the expectancy or probability held by the individuaI that his own Behavior cannOt dEtermine the occurrence of the outcomes, or reinforcement he seeks (page 784; emphas1s in originEI). In short, the concepts of Perceived Efficacy and Locus of Control are Shown to result in feelings of powerlessness and alienation, under certain circumstances. The problems of Anomie and Alienation, examined a century ago by Durkheim and Marx, seem not to have lost their relevancy for discussions of the current human and social condition. gglationship between Primary Relations and Psychological Equilibrium Participation in a primary group is at least a necessary condition for the achievement and maintenance of psychological equilibrium. The reference to the achievement of psychological equilibrium implicates the complex dynamics of the development of personality, while the maintenance of psychological equilibrium refers to successful coping with the problems of daily life. The two processes are not independent of each other; maintenance assumes the prior achievement of relative psychological equilibrium, while .' ‘ rqe A, one 3““ ‘ . RAQSA v Vvoa V-‘ (as we: cope}. 20 the achievement of equilibrium is strengthened and consolidated with the successful c0ping with daily life (as well as being subject to degradation with failure to cope). Role of Primary Group Membership in Achievement of Psychological Equilibrium: Anomie Membership in primary groups is a necessary, though not sufficient, condition for the achievement of psychological equilibrium. Sociological and psychological evidence is quite persuasive with regard to this hypothesis. Cooley (1962), with whom the concept of primary groups originated, states that primary groups are: ". . . primary in several senses, but chiefly in that they are fundamental in forming the social nature and ideals of the individual (page 23). For the individual to internalize the "rules of social life," or, more broadly, for him to internalize social norms regarding the ends to be sought and the means by which he may seek these ends, he must experience the moral force of these norms. Secondary relations do not contain the potential for this experience. In other words, if it is the social existence of man that determines his consciousness, as asserted by Marx (1971), then his "social existence" must provide the experiences from which he abstracts social norms of behavior, and makes them part of his consciousness. 2 . . It 15 not be1ng suggested that this abstraction and internalization is a conscious process, nor that these norms ex1st 1n a form that can be Verbalized. I “a..- 50%! one . “'~. dzst...-- . is being b in CC: baba‘ from the environ: 50th c but an c general] enticed But the nCtice E the rela Young c3 {Elatio; 21 Now one way to conceptualize social existence is to distinguish in it primary from secondary relations. What is being suggested here is that secondary relations do not contain the potential for the necessary experiences because they do not have a hold over the individual strong enough for him to feel the weight of the sanctions that follow deviance from the norms. Implicit in the above statements is a model of human behavior that assumes that individuals seek positive outcomes from their interactions with objects and people in their environment. Which positive outcomes are most intently sought depends on a host of subjective and objective factors, but an outcome that is desired in early childhood and generally continues to be desired in adulthood is to be noticed and accepted (Jersild, Telford, and Sawrey, 1975). But the significant point for this diScussion is that such notice and acceptance is differentially valued depending on the relationship between the individuals concerned. Even the young child discriminates between primary and secondary relationships by showing considerably different levels of need for notice and acceptance from mother as compared to, say, the postman. Sullivan (1953) has developed quite fully the point that a child is not affected equally by all the people with whom he comes in contact. A child's self- evaluations are influenced dominantly by the "significant others" in his social world. The child is born into a given culture with criteria and standards of worth already defined F“ (I f) .4 (D L‘ n . Hm r4. .‘2’ ‘3; 22 and established. From the moment of birth, the child is subject to a set of built-in rewards and punishments for being or failing to be a certain type of person and for doing or not doing certain things. He comes to evaluate himself and his worth as an individual in terms of these social reinforcements. The point to be emphasized is that these social reinforcements are effective only if they come from "significant othersf'that is, from people who have primary relations with the child. In the complex interaction between parents and child, the person not only learns about the conventions and standards that govern social life, but he also internalizes them, that is, incorporates them in his own personality. The process by which values and specific rules are internalized is complex, but it is usually tied closely to the intimate relations between children and parents (Sullivan, 1953, page 336; emphasis added). All development does not occur in the parent-child relationship, of course. Later experiences with peer groups and with other adults also have Profound implications for the achievement of psychological equilibrium, but in all of these cases as well, it is from primary relations that the greatest influence flows. It is not being suggested that having primary relations is sufficient in itself for the achievement of psychological equilibrium, only that it is necessary. Furthermore, primary relations do not guarantee the development of socially adaptive personalities. For example, Haimowitz (1966) has developed the hypothesis that the only way a person can become a professional criminal is by getting the idea that he is expected to be ‘- CaKHC . Y.” . .5 kid: ‘ 5r 23 an outlaw by thoseowhom he takes seriously: his parents, friends, neighbors, or teachers (emphasis added). A great number of forces influence the achievement of a stable and well-adapted personality. There is no intention to discuss all of these here, only to indicate that one necessary condition for the achievement of psychological equilibrium is the existence of primary relations. In the absence of primary relations,experiences in which the worth of the "rules of social life" are impressed upon the consciousness cannot occur. The resulting paucity of internalized norms is what we have identified as anomie. Role of Primary Group Membership in Achievement of Psychological Equilibrium: Alienation Bronfenbrenner (1972) expresses a sense of alarm about the social and political apathy of the majority of students surveyed by Yankelovich (1972). While perceiving gross injustice in the major institutions of their society, most students are apparently not motivated to do very much about it. Bronfenbrenner suggests that this apathy is caused by feelings of powerlessness and defeat which result from a decrease, over a 25-year period, in all Spheres of interaction between parents and children. Among the causes of this decline are changes in the structure of the family-- working mothers, single-parent families--and the influence of television. Bronfenbrenner quotes from the report prepared for the White House Conference on Children (Report to the President, 1970): ‘0! :1 m If {1' t .‘i‘..- 24 The primary danger of the television screen lies not so much in the behavior it produces as the behavior it prevents-~the talks, the games, the family festivities and arguments through which much of the child's learning takes place and his character is formed. Turning on the television set can turn off the process that transforms children into peOple. We may mention briefly here the observation in the introduc- tion by Rieff to the 1962 reprint of Cooley's Social Organization that the TV.penetrates even the bedroom: "Television in the bedroom helps the members of even that sticky primary group [to] ignore each other" (page xiv). The clue to the process by which this decline in interaction in a primary group results in the apathy and feelings of powerlessness of youth is contained in Bronfenbrenner's suggestion that it is primarily through observing, playing, and working with others older and younger than himself that a child discovers HEEE.EE gag £2 and 292.22 can become (emphasis added). This sense of control over actions and over one's personality will not develop in an environment that does not provide the individual an opportunity to actively manipulate his physical and social environment, and to learn from the consequences of such activity. Clearly, secondary relation- ships do not provide such an environment, both because of the paucity of interactions in which the individual attempts to influence the other person's behavior, as well as because of the relatively lower value of social reinforcement obtained from secondary sources. The lack of a sense of personal efficacy that results from a childhood in which But-1 1,. k 910B; relat well- n xv 1.1.... Q— I_L._n c-L 25 secondary relationships predominate may result, as suggested earlier (page 19), in feelings of powerlessness and apathy. Ritchie and Koller (1964) emphasize the time element as the reason for the importance of the primary group: The longer the exposure to others, the greater the impact . . . the less the exposure to others, or if the relationship is fleeting, passing, or momentary, as is true of many secondary groups, then the more shallow are the results in personality-building. . . . It is true that secondary-group situations also build personalities, but by comparison with [primary group] experiences, they are less profound in their impact and can be more easily nullified by other secondary and especially primary group circumstances (pages 77—78). The emphasis on the time element is especially significant to this discussion, because stability of employment is the most important of the characteristics of work organizations that will be hypothesized to be related to emotional well- being (see page 80). Role of Primary Group Membership in the Maintenancethe translation referenced here, suggests that, "like many an eminent thinker" Tdnnies was a man of one central idea which he develOped in its various ramifications in practically all his theoretical works. This central idea is his theory of Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft as two different modes of mentality and behavior, and as two different types of society. Sorokin goes on to point out that the idea is much older than Tannies, and is contained in the work of Confucius, Plato, Aristotle, Cicero, St. Augustine de 47 48 Flore, St. Aquinas, and many others: . . . The Gemeinschaft type of mentality was extolled by Confucius: Confucius' theory of the five fundamental social relationships of father and son, elder brother and younger, husband and wife, ruler and subject, friend and friend closely resembles Toennies' main Gemeinschaft ties between mother and child, father and children, sisters and brothers, friends and friends, and rulers and subjects. Confucius not only unfolded the Gemeinschaft type of society but also set it off agaihst the Gesellschaft type . . . Plato in his Republic and Laws likewise gave a full portrait of both types of society, as well as of human personality. His ideal republic, especially the personality and social regime of the Guardians, is clearly and definitely of the Gemeinschaft type, while his detailed picture of the oligarchic or capitalistic society and man is a conspicuous example of the Gesellschaft type. Aristotle and, after him, Cicero, ih their analyses of the true and false friendship, gave us in clear-cut form the classical outlines of the two types. The same types are found running through the works of the Church fathers, especially those of St. Augustine. Here the theory of the Church and the 'City of God' as the corpus mysticum of Gemeinschaft type is-contrasted to the 'society of man' depicted along the lines of the Gesellschaft type. Throughout the writings of the great medieval thinkers like Joachim de Fiore, Albertus Magnus, St. Thomas Aquinas, Nicolaus Cusanus, and others the dichotomy persists . . . it is also the central idea of Ibn Khaldun, in History of Berbers and in his Prolegomenes to the Universal History. . . . In Germany partiCularly, as Gierke has clearly shown, the Gemeinschaft type was deeply rooted in the very soil of the Teutonic culture . . . immediately preceding the appearance of Toennies' work both types of society and personality were well depicted by many a German thinker, beginning with the leaders of the Historical School, Savigny and Puchta, and ending with Hegel, whose 'Family- Society' and 'Civic Society' are almost twins of Toennies' Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft. (T6nnies, 1940, pages vii-viii). St. Augustine's distinction between the "City of God" and "the society of man" is especially relevant in the light of 49 Durkheim's distinction between the "sacred" and the "profane." In varied forms, the categories continued to function in the writings of the social thinkers of the later centuries. Thus, Hegel's "Family-Society" and Civic Society" are almost twins of Tdnnies' Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft, according to Sorokin. Weber's interest in the "rationalization" of human life and society may be seen as a continuation of this idea. More recently, Stark (1979) has documented some current manifestations of the Great Transformation. What is the theme that has fascinated so many thinkers? In the following pages, a few of the more recent scholars who have documented and commented upon the Great Transformation will be discussed. Of the authors mentioned by Sorokin, only Tonnies will be included in the present discussion. Weber's interest in rationalization, Stinchcombe's views on the type of people needed in the task of administration of modern industrial organizations, Warren's documentation of the changes in American communities, and the ideas of Cooley, Wirth and other sociologists are Shown to be pointing to the same phenomenon that Tonnies wrote about. T6nnies Tdnnies documents a historical shift, over centuries, of societies from being of the Gemeinschaft type to being of the Gesellschaft type. The essence of the distinction between Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft is contained in the basis for the relationships that predominate in these two 50 types. All social.relationships are created by human will, but this will may vary from one Situation to another.‘iThe distinction between rational will and natural will was mentioned earlier (page 8). When a relationship is constituted in order to achieve a specific purpose, regard- less of the sentiments of the parties to the relationship, it is said to be based on rational will. If people associate with each other "merely" for the pleasure of association, the relationship is said to be based on natural will: the relationship is not a means to an end, but an end in itself. In the Gemeipschaft, natural will forms the basis for the relationship between men as well as between man and his "life activity," his work. Thus, the calling of the peasant, the hunter, craftsman, or the artist is a way of life, not merely a means to an end. In Gesellschaft, rational will is the predominant basis for social relationships and for man's relationship to work. Marx's (1963) observation, that alienated work is work which is not the satisfaction of a need but only a means for satisfying other needs, may be recalled here. Tdnnies suggests that when peOple are used as mere means to ends, even as "inanimate things," such usage is governed by rational will. There can be little doubt that this is Buber's (1952) I-It relationship. T6nnies is careful to point out that the contrast is not between rational will and non-rational will, because: . . . intellect and reason belong to natural will as well as to rational will. Indeed, intellect t: 51 in natural will attains its fruition in the creative, formative and artistic ability and works (page 17). The importance of the collective conscience in Gemeinschaft is indicated in the following passage: . . . in Gemeinschaft, [individuals] remain essentially uniEediin spite of all separating factors, whereas in the Gesellschaft they are essentially separated in spite of ail uniting factors. In the Gesellschaft, as contrasted with the Geminschaft, we find no actions that can be derived from an a priori and necessarily existing unity; no actions,_t erefore, which manifest the will and the spirit of the unity even if performed by the individual; no actions which, insofar as they are performed by the individual, take place on behalf of those united with him (page 74). T310211 The "engine" of this Great Transformation, according to Tonnies, is the development of trade. Introduction of large-scale trade into the integrated.communities of agrarian and town societies, with the attendant desire for profitable use of money, liquidated the old ideologies and brought about the capitalistic age with its rationalistic intellectual attitude. The relationship between the growth of "rationalism" and the growth of capitalism is most thoroughly maintained by Weber. Giddens (1971) finds the concept of rationalization entering into so many of Weber's historical writings that elucidation of the main Spheres of its application is difficult. For Weber, the Great Transformation manifests itself in the growth of rationalization--or in Tdnnies' terms, the increasing use of rational will in all social 52 affairs. Contrary to Tonnies, however, Weber is unwilling to attribute this growth to any single cauSe like trade, for he sees rationalization as both cause and consequence not only of trade but of a host of other factors as well. Nonetheless, Weber is convinced that, (1) the possibility of rational calculation of profits and losses in terms of money: (2) the use of a technology constructed and organized on the basis of rational principles, and (3) the separation of home and work place are all among the conditions essential to modern capitalistic enterprise. Weber is also convinced that rational legal administration, that is, bureaucracy, as well as democracy, are also intimately linked both to rationalization and capitalism. Weber was of the opinion that increasing bureaucratization of the modern world was inevitable. The increased role of rational will in all social affairs implicated by this cannot fail to undermine the role of natural will: The specific character (of bureaucracy), which is welcomed by capitalism, develops the more completely the more the bureaucracy is 'dehumanized,‘ the more completely it succeeds in eliminating from official business, love, hatred, and all purely personal, irrational and emotional elements which escape calcualation (Weber quoted by Giddens, 1971, page 216). In other words, the more societies modernize and industrialize, the more society is dominated by rational will and the more social relations take on the character of secondary relations. Nor is this true only of capitalist 53 societies; Weber is convinced that such bureaucratization and rationalization is unavoidable in socialist societies as well. The functional specialization demanded by bureaucratization heralds for Weber the demise of the "universal" man, the "thorough and complete human being" whom Durkheim speaks of. It is the conception men have of themselves as means to an and rather than as ends in themselves in the modern society that is at the root of weber's concern. In Weber's eyes, according to Giddens (1971, page 235): The progression of bureaucratization increasingly reveals a tension between the demand for technical efficiency of administration on the one hand, and the human values of spontaneity and autonomy [values based on natural will] on the other. The main normative issue, in Weber's view, is not how the process of bureaucratization can become reversed, because that is impossible in a society which requires calculative precision in the admin- istration of its various institutions; 'the great question thus is . . . what can we set against this mechanization to preserve a certain section of humanity from this fragmentation of the soul, this complete ascendancy of the bureaucratic ideal of life?' What one finds in Weber, in other words, is the proposition that bureaucratization of society leads to a fundamental change in man, a change in which man moves from being a "whole" to a fragment, a change from universal man to functional specialist. Such a fundamental change occurs, of course, because interpersonal relationships have changed from a predominance of primary to secondary characteristics. Jacoby (1973), in discussing the F .- 3 : . Ca .3 .5 2. hi. de de ac” tr ne 54 "social effect" of bureaucracy, quotes Ruhle-Gerstel as follows: Neurosis is not a sickness, but a way of life. In neurosis the separation of the individual from his sense of association (Gemeinscnaft) with other individuals becomes a distinguiShing category of the mind. Neumann (1944) relates bureaucracy directly to the form that human relations take: Bureaucratization, correctly understood, is a process operating in both public and private spheres, in the state as well as in society. It means that human relations lose their directness and become mediated relations in whiEh third parties, publ1c or private functionaries seated more or less securely in power, authoritatively prescribe the behavior of man (emphasis added). Stinchcombe Stinchcombe (1974) makes essentially the same point in his discussion of the social sources of individual rationality. Stinchcombe notes Weber's emphasis on the degree to which rational administration or bureaucracy depends on the differentiation of role behavior in administration from familial, local, and friendship ties; that is, the predominance of rational will rather than natural will. Stinchcombe argues that, to the degree that people are appropriately socialized for work in modern administrative structures, they Should be personally inclined to differentiate their administrative activity from "primordial" ties. Furthermore, an important component of individual rationality--the inclination to abstract--is tied to the decline of primordial motivations. The essence istrat; expense family, ascends decline decline 55 of Stinchcombe's argument is this: if life's experiences are to be interpreted in terms of abstractions like the past and the future as is required in the admin- istration of bureaucracy, then this can only occur at the expense of interpretation in terms of primordial ties of family, neighborhood, and age groups. Once again then, the ascendancy of rational will in society is related to the decline of the sources of primary relations, to the decline of the importance of natural will. Warren One may mention briefly Maine's famous observation, referred to by Warren (1972), that there is a historical change of social relations from those based on family relations--a situation in which status in the family was of central concern--to those based on.the individual's relationships to others outside the family which are formalized in the nature of a contract. Warren (1972) himself has documented the "great change in American communities" in "recent decades" in terms that leave little doubt as to its identity with the Great Transformation being discussed here. More relevant to the proposition that this change is associated with a weakening of the traditional sources of primary relations is Warren's observation that the change toward interdependence and the proliferation of individualized functions that accompanies the division of labor has resulted in fewer of these functions being performed within the individual family. CCIT'RLIEL s'v-ar 4' a. ‘ 8‘ COMET. ’4 m lhi - H‘ 56 The family becomes less significant as a locus for recreational and service functions, as well as thoSe of economic production. The division of labor is also at the root of the structural differentiation of American communities, whiCh has weakened the contribution that shared occupation and occupational interests make toward community cohesion. The shift from reliance on primary relations to reliance on secondary relations that is implied by this differentiation has "profound" implications for the individual and the community. Warren (1972) quotes Linton as follows: The modern city, with its multiplicity of organiza- tions of every conceivable sort, presents the picture of a mass of individuals who have lost their bands and who are trying, in uncertain and fumbling fashion, to find some substitute. New types of grouping based on congeniality, business association, or community of interests are springing up on all Sides, but nothing has so far appeared which seems capable of taking over the primary functions of the local group as these relate to individuals. Membership in the Rotary Club is not an adequate substitute for friendly neighbors (page 61). For the community, the "great change" results in the decline of the neighborhood as a Significant social unit in both rural and urban areas. Since the neighborhood's significance as a social unit is precisely in its potential as a locus for primary-group relationships, the outcome of division of labor and differentiation of interests is a weakening of the sources of primary relations. The decline of the family and neighborhood as a locus for recreational, service, and economic functions leaves all these functions to be performed by specialized agencies, (An-4‘. r] entity for ex ment's impers. GIOWth the $01 increag relatic 0n SOurceS fOr the MARY of neighbo with pr USually Placed t° Perf 57 private as well as public. Richmond (1979) sees in this the essential engine for the phenomenal growth in local and federal government. Jacoby (1973) makes essentially the same point: Once capitalism transformed the traditional way of life, factors such as the effectiveness of competi- tion, freedom of movement, and the absence of any system of social security compelled the state to assume responsibility for the protection and welfare of the individual (page 147). The state is, of course, a more distant and abstract entity than the community. 'For many reasonS--including, for example, its intention to be equitable-~the govern- ment's dealings with its citizens can be expected to be impersonal, universal, and formal. In other words, in the growth of government can be seen, as cause, a decline in the sources of primary relations and, as a consequence, an increase in the sources of, and necessity for, secondary relations. One may also speculate that this loss of traditional sources of socialization is at least partly reSponsible for the decline in the performance of the school system. Many of the functions once performed by the family and neighborhood now fall upon the school system. Combined with prohibitions against "moral education" that are usually imposed on school systems, the increased demands placed on the system may partly account for its inability to perform its most basic functions. econom: Cooley causes same 50 urban 3. Of pepuL group 1; the Way listed h shift fr Paradox PSYCholo. thSiCal Th1 Of emc 399 COu dis to The fom‘aliz mode rm 1 58 Egoleyy Wirth, Angell, Kasarda and JanOWitz Cooley's (1962) observation, as early as 1909, that the intimacy of the neighborhood has been broken up by the growth of an intricate mesh of wider contacts which "leaves us strangers to pe0p1e who live in the same house" has been mentioned before (see page 17). The result is a diminished economic and spiritual community with one's neighbors. Cooley points to commerce and industry as the leading causes of this phenomenon. Wirth (1938) begins with the same forces and suggests that the essential character of urban society results from an increased level and density of population, and the heterogeneity of individual and group life. These, in turn, lead to a fundamental change in the way people relate to each other. The characteristics listed by Wirth leave no doubt that he is talking about a Shift from primary relations to secondary relations. The paradox of people further apart from each other psychologically and emotionally, living closer together physically, has perverse consequences: This close living together and working together of individuals who have no sentimental and emotional ties fosters a spirit of competition, aggrandizement, and mutual exploitation. To counteract irresponsibility and potential disorder, formal controls tend to be resorted to (page 21). The last sentence is significant in its suggestion that formalized mechanisms of control become imperative in modern, industrial-urban societies. Essentially the same 59 suggestion was made by Jacoby (1973), Quoted earlier (see page 56). Warren (1972) too, has suggested that there is an increase in the formalized and impersonal mechanisms of control to compensate for the loss of influence of the community over individual behavior brought about by the decline of the community, in T6nnies'GemeinSchaft sense of the word. Angell (1951, 1974) is one of the few scholars who has attempted to "measure" the moral integration of American cities. His use of welfare and crime indices in this connection has been mentioned before (see page 35). In a study of 43 cities conducted in 1940, Angell found that heterogeneity and mobility accounted for 62 percent of the variance among the cities in moral integration. Angell suggested that metropolitan growth is accompanied by the following developments: (1) increase in size and complexity of all kinds of bureaucracies, thereby adding to alienation and apathy; (2) the tendency for life to become more impersonal and unrewarding; (3) reduced thoroughness in the internalization of norms, as the interests of family members become more distant in space and more divergent in character; and (4) the reduced ability of both formal and informal social controls to maintain public order. After a follow- up measurement in 1970, Angell (1974) concludes that the moral integration of large American urban communities was "seriously impaired" during the period 1940 to 1970. 5-“ E! ‘f 'I‘ U: 60 Finally, Kasarda and Janowitz (1974) suggest that length of residence is a central and crucial factor in the development of the social bonds of kinship. The suggestion that mobility is inversely related to the development and maintenance of primary relations is contained in many of the writings discussed above. An examination of Table 1 (page 11) makes evident that length of residence should be expected to be a crucial factor in the development of primary relations, not only because of the time involved in the development of such associations, but also because participants in a relationship WOUId be less likely to fear loss of psychological investment when they expect each other to "stay;put." Cooley's (1962) observation that primary relations form a comparatively permanent "anchor" to which the more transient secondary relations are attached brings to mind the image of the child who, having recently learned to walk, ventures a few feet away from his mother, never letting her be lost from his line of vision, yet, even with that, returning joyfully every few minutes to briefly touch and be "in the presence," before toddling off again. It remains to be suggested that industrialization and modernization are accompanied by increased mobility. The idea that mobility is a pre-condition for rapid industrialization is so well-established, at least in the Western developed countries, that mobility is often used as an index of industrialization. In a more 61 theoretical vein, Stinchcombe (1974) suggests that effective industrial administrators are "cosmopolitans" who are characterized by a detachment from the local system. One factor in the development of such detachment is the mobility of cosmopolitans, the fact that they travel more. Jennings (1967) has developed extensive theoretical notions of the role of an individual's career mobility in his success in modern American business. Jennings became interested in mobility when he discovered that the career history of the most successful executives in the country's largest corporations invariably consisted of a great deal of mobility. These men rarely spent more than two or three years in the same position. Originally, this mobility was merely a result of the fact that corporations grew faster than they could develop the talent necessary to fill the staffing needs to sustain this growth. This incidental mobility was converted to a required mobility, however, when it was discovered that it led to the development of extremely competent managers. Jennings believes that mobility results in competence because of the larger variety of experiences and problems encountered by the mobile manager. In addition to the role mobility plays in the development of talent, one may expect that the ability of organizations to rapidly adjust the size of the labor force in response to changes in the environment would enhance their efficiency. In the absence of legal 62 prohibitions against the use of this strategy of adjusting the labor force, we may expect industrial organizations to use it, thereby contributing to the mobility in society. For whatever causes, the census statistics of mobility leave no doubt that America is a country "on the move." Lansing and Mueller (1967) call geographic mobility a "basic characteristic" of the population of the U.S. At the time of their report as much as 20 percent of the civilian population one year old and over were reported to have moved in the last year. Of these, close to 7 percent moved across county lines. Data collected in 1960 revealed that almost half the population of the country moved to a new address within a five-year period. More significantly, the reasons for moving are largely economic: in 1963, 72 percent of the movers reported that they moved for purely economic or partly economic reasons, such as taking up a job, looking for work, or job transfer. Especially Significant for this study is the statistic that the "Average Duration of Employment" for the whole U.S. industrial economy is about two years (Statistical Abstract of the U.S., 1970, page 218). In other words, the average annual labor force turnover for the whole industrial-commercial economy is 50 percent. As much as half of the labor force presently employed will not be working at the same location next year, for voluntary or involuntary reasons. While information about the median duration of employment, if it were available, might temper 63 the conclusion being made here, there can nonetheless be little doubt that mobilityis indeed a basic characteristic of American society. Summary In conclusion, there is an impressive array of evidence and thought that suggests that it is the very processes that are essential to industrialization and modernization that result in the erosion of sources of primary relations in societies. While the discussion here has focused on recent history, and therefore on industriali- zation, the Great Transformation is presented as a continuous movement of Western societies, over many centuries, from being of the "collective type" towards being of the "individual type." The discussion here is not meant to belittle the benefits to humanity of industrializa- tion, nor is there any suggestion that the writer wishes for a return to a previous form of social organization. On the contrary, the thrust of the next chapter is to locate a mechanism whereby the benefits of industrialization may be enjoyed by humanity without concurrent declines in individual and societal health. Proposition 4 may be viewed as a version of the Great Transformation: As societies modernize and industrialize, the traditional sources of primary relations weaken. mainten validit' 'n‘: re d in the 64 Primary relations had earlier been shown to be essential to the achievement and maintenance of psychological equilibrium by individuals, and to the achievement and maintenance of sociological equilibrium. If there is any validity to these suggestions, then the urgent question is: Where do we go from here? This is the question addressed in the following chapter. CHAPTER IV ORGANIZATIONAL PRIMARY RELATIONS AS A DETERMINANT OF SOCIETAL, ORGANIZATIONAL , AND EMPLOYEE HEALTH Without work all Life goes rotten But when work is Soulless, life Stifles and dies. Albert Camus PrOposition 5 contains an answer to the question posed at the end of the last chapter. This chapter elaborates on Proposition 5, which states that work organizations may be able to provide an environment in which primary relations can develop. Implicit in the discussion above is the proposition that modern, industrial societies must locate and develop substitutes for the traditional sources of primary relationships. Homans (1950) is emphatic in asserting that democratic civilizations cannot survive without the values of the small group. At the level of the small group, society has always been able to cohere . . . if civiliza- tion is to stand, it must maintain, in the relation between the groups that make up society and the central direction of society, some of the features of the small group itself. If we do not solve this problem, the effort to achieve our most high-minded 65 66 purposes may lead us not to UtOpia but to Byzantium (page 468). ‘ Fromm's (1941) conclusion is that civilization may fall Simply because men will not tolerate loss of primary group association for long. Fromm says that in the last 400 years men have been gradually set free from the restraints of traditional society. But in losing these restraints, they have also lost the sense of belongingness to a group whose members cooperate in securing the deepest interests of each. If freedom is to mean no more than emotional isolation, it will not survive. Men will do "any mad thing, even merge in a mass under the sword of a tyrant," to escape from freedom of this kind. Certainly, recent history contains numerous events that fit this description. This chapter begins to provide a partial response to this problem. The impetus for the suggestions contained here, and indeed for the dissertation as a whole, comes from the work of Ouchi and his colleagues (Ouchi, 1981; Ouchi and Johnson, 1978; Ouchi and Price, 1978). Presently constituted groups in society that may contain the potential for becoming a source of primary relations are first located. The work organization is shown to be ideal in this regard. The question of how the potential may be actualized then leads to an examination of alternate forms of organization of these groups in non-western cultures, specifically China and Japan. The Japanese form of work organization is found to be of special interest. Characteristics of Japanese work organizations that appear to be related to their 67 ability to be a source of primary relations are examined, with a view to locating those characteristics that are not culture-bound, and hence may be applicable to the American Situation. The Potential of Work Groups What alternatives exist for the replacement of traditional sources of primary relations? What kinds of groups exist in modern, industrial societies which have the potential for the develOpment of primary relationships, that is, which involve people in continuous, face-to-face interaction over many years? Clearly, one such group is the work group. Durkheim's (1933) own prescription was what he called the occupational group, in which members of a particular occupation would unite and organize into a single body. This group would function as a locus for the formation of close relationships among members, resulting in the effective regulation of industry or profession, and contributing to the moral solidarity of the society. The similarity of Durkheim's occupational‘ group to today's trade or professional organization is striking, and the investigation of whether or not professional organizations perform the functions Durkheim expected occupational groups to perform would be worthwhile. One suspects, however, that the occupational groups Durkheim had in mind would be considerably smaller and much more localized geographically than the modern-day professional associations. Contact among members of 68 professional associations seems too intermittent and too purposive (i.e., rationally motivated) to result in the kinds of relationships that would contain the potential for moral regulation. Nonetheless, the contribution of professional associations to the "moral regulation" of member behavior is evidenced by the "codes of ethics" that exist in the older and well-established associations. Work organizations often result in close, face-to- face interactions of the same peOple over long periods of time. But work organizations, too, bring people together around a purpose, and may be expected to be dominated by relationships based on rational will; Such was the conviction of students of organizations in the decades before Roethlisberger and Dickson, and Mayo. Now it is known, of course, that no work organization exists without an extensive informal organization that influences profoundly the behavior of the members of the organization. But more significant for our present discussion is the prOposition that the formal organization may be structured in ways which promote the likelihood of the organization becoming a locus for primary relations. In his introduction to Mayo's (1975) "The Social Problems of an Industrial Civilization," Donham notes: Mayo shows us for the first time in the form of specific instances that it is within the power of industrial administrators to create within industry itself a partially effective substitute for the old stabilizing effect of the neighborhood. Given stable employment, it might make of industry a socially satisfying 69 way of life as well as a way of making a _ living (page xiv). _ - ' Mayo finds the social mechanism that would achieve the necessary social integration in the "working group." Mayo found the roots of this solution to the problem in traditional societies, where the social order is maintained not by a direct tie between each individual and the state, but rather by the relationship of each individual to a clan, a smaller, primary group. All clans were then related to the larger government of the state. Mayo further observed that in modern industry management is not related to Single workers but always to working groups. Here, Mayo is referring not only to formal work groups, but also to the informal groups that inevitably form in departments that do not call for formal work groups as such. Thus, Mayo concludes that the fundamental objective of management must be to aid the creation and maintenance of stable, effective working groups within the enterprise. In the Opinion of Ouchi and Price (1978), "all contemporary efforts at organizational development can be said to conform to this basic principle" (page 29). Thus, one finds in the works of McGregor (1960, 1966) and Likert (1961, 1967) the consistent assertion that the development of cohesive small groups that are linked to each other leads to organizational success, and that the focus of organizational development efforts should be the development of interpersonal Skill. This latter prescription is also found in Argyris (1964, 1974). 7O Ouchi and Price (1978) review the work of the Human Relations school, represented by the authors mentioned above, and conclude that the fundamental propositions that seem to underlie this school of thought may be stated as follows: (a) The hierarchical Structure (a system of superior-subordinate relationships based on legitimate authority, close monitoring of activity, job specialization, and individual responsibility) is the predominant form of organization in modern, industrial societies. (b) Hierarchical organizations are naturally . and inevitably hostile to the growth needs of individuals, because of the high degree of specialization, the narrow economic measures of performance, and the fostering of a state of dependence of subordinates on superiors. (c) Consequently, steps must be taken to mitigate these negative effects of hierarchy. What Ouchi and Price fail to find in the Human Relations school is an explanation for prOposition (a) above. Why does hierarchy seem to be the inevitable response to industrialization? What essential functions of managerial control are served by hierarchy? Is hierarchy the only structure that serve these functions? w (9' 9“ IE: 71 92.122. A logical place to look for the beginningscxfthe answer to the last question is the typical forms of industrial organization in non-Western cultures. Whyte's (1973) study of modernization in China provides a suggestion to the effect that the hierarchical structure may not be the only way to organize collective effort. The Maoist ideology is, in fact, opposed to bureaucracy on the grounds that it allows the educated to set themselves apart from the rest of the p0pu1ation and to advance themselves and their relatives at the expense of the common man. For the same reason, a high degree of specialization and the development of professionalism is to be discouraged. If allowed, these would lead to "the masses [feeling] incapable of affecting the decisions affecting their lives," leading to diScouragement and lack of dedication. This would undermine whatever decisions their superiors would make. Superiors should, therefore, be less concerned about establishing their professional prerogatives and more concerned about forging close ties with their subordinates. Individuals are supposed to obey, not because they are subordinate in a legitimate organization or because they have less technical knowledge than their Superiors, but because of their commitment to the goals of the group. This commitment is to be the outcome of group solidarity. Considerable emphasis is placed upon minimizing differences between superiors and subordinates, o 72 through similarity of dress, through the location in the same building of the more spacious apartment of the administrator and the smaller residence of his unit's janitor, and through "mass line" procedures, in which managerial cadres regularly Spent part of their time out of their offices and working with their hands alongside subordinates. The basic vehicle for the participation of the masses in decision-making are the discussion groups--the factory work group, the school row, the office section, the military squad--all of which are to maintain their form outside the activities and hours of the formal organizational life. PeOple are expected to treat each other not just as holders of narrow roles, but as whole individuals with problems and private lives which may affect organizational performance. Individual performance gains that result from wider differentials and status distinctions--basic tools of the motivational trade in Western organizations-~are held by the Maoists to be more than offset by lowered organizational cohesion and morale. Thus, after extensive interviews with captured Chinese soldiers during the Korean War, George (1967) finds that much of the impressive performance of the Chinese Army against the more heavily armed U.N. forces could be attributed to the encapsulation of soldiers in small groups with regular political rituals and group criticisms, and the 73 maintenance of comradely relations and mutual consultation between officers and men. The Maoist alternative to hierarchy may thus be seen to be the small group or the "clan." There is, however, an essential difference between the "cohesive work group" sought by the Human Relations school and the "discussion group" of China. The Human Relations school does not see the cohesive group as serving the control functions that the discussion group is to serve; that is, it does not see the cohesive group as an alternative to hierarchy. The Maoist system seeks to obtain organizationally required behaviors through ensuring that individuals, because they value their membership in the group, will be committed to the goals of the group. Japan A significant segment of Japanese industry shares some of the characteristics of the Maoist system, yet is radically different in other characteristics. For the Western observer, the most striking characteristic of Japanese industrial organization is the Nenko, or lifetime employment system, prevalent in large businesses. Abegglen (1958), who first introduced Western readers to the Japanese factory, calls this the "critical" difference between Japanese and U.S. work organization: When comparing the social organization of the factory in Japan and the United States one difference is immediately noted and continues to dominate and represent much of the total difference between the two systems. At whatever level of organization in the Japanese factory, the worker commits 74 himself on entrance to the company for the remainder of his working career. The company will not discharge him even temporarily except in the most extreme circumstances. He will not quit the company for industial employment elsewhere. He is a member of the company in a way resembling that in which persons are members of families, . . . and other intimate and personal groups in the U.S. (page 11). The yearly turnover quoted by Abegglen for individual firms in the 1949-1953 period ranges from a tenth of a percent to 2 and 3 percent for men, and 10 percent for women. The contrast with an average national turnover rate of up to 50 percent in the U.S.A. (see page above) is striking. The Western relationship is more nearly "contractual," that is, dominated by economic factors or "rational will," whereas the Japanese policy tacitly recognizes that the relationship between the company and the worker is not simply a function of the economic convenience of the two parties. Loyalty to the group and an interchange of responsibilities--a system of shared obligation--takes the place of the economic basis of employment of worker by the firm (Abegglen, 1958, page 17). The policy of lifetime employment may be expected to significantly influence the kind and intensity of relationships that will form among members of the organization. One can imagine the experience of a new employee in a large organization in Japan who is introduced to his fellow employees and who realizes that these are the people who will be his colleagues for the rest of his working life. In contrast, the entrant in a Western 75 counterpart would be aware that the peOple he associates with today may be gone tomorrow, or that he himself may leave. Not only do peOple have insufficient time to develop broad ties in this setting, they would also be motivated to protect themselves against the costs of losing friends by not forming friendships. We may expect, then, the stability 2; employment to be an important determinant of whether or. not the work organization is experienced by employees as a locus for primary relations. In this context, the conclusions of Hall et al. (1970) and Hall and Schneider (1972) from their research in various U.S. organizations are of interest. They were interested in the correlates of "identification" which they view as the "process by which the goals of the organization and those of the individual become increasingly integrated or congruent." They found that tenure (length of service) and organizational identification were significantly (p < .01) related in the forest service and with priests that they studied. They explain this relationship by suggesting the operation within individuals of such factors as (l) increas- ing investment the person makes in the organization, and (2) the effect of foregoing outside Opportunities. Note, however, that Katz (1978) finds that "overall satisfactiona me mm be as am we so am one omnno>< o.- s.o me m.m~ m.~m H.oa mm a.e Amos. ooa>som no sesame .o>a a s o as as a m a nosooooanom no .02 cos owe can ooea mom ems ooom «an noosoedem no .02 some mead mama .aoaH mass mmaa some mass as oosnaaonunm ma.oo A..oo ma.oo ma.oo ee.oo ma.oo ma.oo He.oo mmHZ€mSOU Hamidm 930m¢ ZOHBdZmOmZH M>HBmHmUmmo m mnmfla 116 respondents with a college degree. While most of these companies began as small, privately owned establishments, all except two are presently Subsidiaries of larger corporations. There seemed to be considerable variation in the extent of autonomy from the parent corporation with which the companies Operated. There was, as well, considerable variation in the extent of identification with the parent corporation expressed by the top officers of the companies. Officers of one company expressed, with some sadness, the conviction that it is difficult in today's environment to survive without corporate affiliation. This conviction was not Shared by officers of another company, which remains not only privately held, but also non-unionized. All other companies were unionized. Two had recently had considerable difficulties with contract negotations, including strike action. ' The size of the companies, in terms of number of employees, ranged from 180 to 2,000, with a median size of 350. The overall average length of service of respondents was 18.7 years, but there was considerable variation among companies with respect to average length of service of respondents, ranging from a low of 6.7 years to a high of 32.5 years. The respondent with the shortest length of service had been with the company only three months; the respondent with the longest length of service had just finished 58 years with the same company. 117 While there seems to be somewhat of a pattern in the company values for average length of service and average age of respondents, the relatively low variance in average age and the paradoxical values for the two variables in company 1 prevent us from making any inferences. The management of the companies in the sample would seem to fit the male-dominated stereotype of American industry: none of the t0p officers of the companies in the sample were female, and very few of the middle managerial positions were filled by females. The relatively low percentage of respondents with college degrees may also be noted. Some observations about the sample can also be made from interview data. Two or three officers of the top management team were interviewed by the writer to obtain information about TYPE. All the companies were affected more or less seriously by the depressed conditions in the automobile industry at the time. For example, almost all the companies had cut back on their blue-collar workforce and "even" some white-collar employees were on lay off. Questions in the interview that dealt with Wholistic Concern and Global Philosophy of Management often drew a blank, suggesting that these are concepts alien to the companies in the sample. Finally, it may be mentioned that the writer was received cordially and fairly punctually by officers of the companies in the sample. The interviewees seemed comfortable and unconstrained during the interviews. 118 Data Generation Procedures The principles that guided the selection and conStruc- tion of data generation procedures may be briefly stated as follows: the "state of the art" of measurement in the social sciences being what it is, the only prudent procedure for data collection is to use multiple tests and multiple methods. Wherever possible, already-developed tests, with known characteristics and relationships to measures of other variables, are desirable. If relatively "objective" measures are available for any of the variables, these should be used. A significant problem in measurement is that of relevance or "non-attitudes" (Sellitz, et a1., 1979), that is, eliciting responses from subjects which do not reflect their views and feelings. Limited response items such as the ones typically used in survey research may tap such "non-attitudes." Semi-projective techniques may reduce this source of "error variance" and should be employed when non-attitudes are likely to be a problem. Finally, independent sources for the different variables are to be preferred if this is at all possible in conceptual and operational terms. From each company in the sample, data were obtained from two groups of respondents. The "top management" group consisted of two or three of the highest ranking officers of the company. Members of this group were interviewed with the purpose of obtaining information on TYPE only. The "middle management" group consisted of 119 managerial employees other than those included in the top management group. Members of the middle management group responded to a survey questionnaire containing a number of scales as described below. Additionally, some members of the tOp management group also consented to respond to the survey. A copy of the survey questionnaire is contained in Appendix A. . Primariness of Individuals' Relationship with the Organization Data on PIRO was obtained from responses of the middle management group to two scales in the survey. These scales are described below. Egg (Primariness-Organization-Sentence Completion) is a lS-item sentence completion scale designed to direct the reSpondent's attention to his/her relationship with the organization. This particular "projective" technique was selected because it has the potential of avoiding the problem of "non-attitudes" while at the same time allowing enough structure to obtain reSponses from the domain of interest. The instructions also attempt to enhance the likelihood of obtaining the desired response set. An initial, longer version was modified and shortened after pilot testing. Coding or rating of the completed sentences was done by two raters working independently of each other. Instructions to the raters of POS are reproduced in Appendix B. 120 P00 (Primariness-Organization Questionnaire) is a 17- item scale that appears directly after POS on the survey. These Likert-type items are based directly upon the Opera- tional definition of primariness of individual's relationship with the organization (PIRO). Some of the items were modeled after items contained in Hemphill's (1956) Group Dimensions Description Questionnaire. One of the dimensions Hemphill uses is called Potency, and is defined as follows: Potency is the degree to which a group has primary significance for its members. It is reflected by the kind of needs which a group is satisfying or has the potentiality of satisfying, by the extent of readjustment which would be required of members should the group fail, and by the degree to which a group has meaning to the members with reference to their central values (page 4). The similarity of this concept to PIRO is obvious. It was found, however, that many of Hemphill's items were inappropriate for PIRO; consequently, only part of the wording of a few of the items were incorporated into POQ. The rest of the items in POQ were developed by the writer. TYPE T£§T_(TYPE-Interview) is a rating of TYPE for each company based on interviews with two or three members of the t0p management group. The tOp management group was employed for this purpose, since organizational character is likely to be more accurately reflected in the perceptions of this group because of its vantage point. In addition, organizational character is more likely to 121 be influenced by the perceptions of this group, for obvious reasons. The average interview lasted about 45 minutes. A schedule of questions was used to provide general direction. The questions were all derived directly from the definition of TYPE. Interviews were taped, and ratings were made directly from the tapes by the writer. A small random sample of the tapes were also rated by an independent rater to provide reliability check. Materials associated with the interview are reproduced in Appendix C. TYPEQ (TYPE-Questionnaire) is a Likert-type scale included in the survey that was filled out by the middle management group. These 14 items were constructed by the writer, based upon the definition of TYPE. TPD (TYPE-Published Data) is a rating for TYPE obtained by calculating an "employment stability" index for each company from information contained in the Directory of Michigan Manufacturers from 1957 to 1978. The directory is published every three or four years, and contains brief descriptions of manufacturing companies in Michigan. The employment stability index was computed by counting the number of officers listed in edition n + 1, who were also listed in edition n, and dividing this number by the total number of officers listed in edition n. A similar measure was employed by Ouchi and Johnson (1978) using information from Dun and Bradstreet publications. TPD is thus an independent measure of TYPE. 122 T§§_(TYPE-Employment Stability) is another measure of "employment stability," this one obtained from information supplied by reSpondents on a "demographic information" questionnaire. One of the items in this questionnaire asked the respondents to indicate the year he/she joined the company. Average length of service of reSpondents from each company were computed from this information. Emotional Well-Being Of the three variables of this study, EWB is closest to being a widely used construct. Consequently, two of the four measures used in this study are standardized measures. §§§_(Emotional Well-Being-Sentence Completion) is a sentence-completion scale constructed by the writer. An initial list of 39 items was modified and reduced to the present version of 15 items after pilot testing. The items are designed to orient the respondent toward his/her general feelings. The semi-projective technique was considered well- suited to the nature of the concept being evaluated. ESC was placed before the other EWB measures to prevent contamination by any defensiveness or discomfort that may be elicited by them. Coding or rating of the completed sentences was done by raters working independently of each other, and independently of the raters for POS. Instructions to the raters of ESC are reproduced in Appendix D. TBAIT is the part of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) that measures Trait Anxiety or Generalized Anxiety 123 (Spielberger, et al, 1970). Trait Anxiety refers to relatively stable individual differences in anxiety proneness, that is, to differences between peOple in the tendency to respond to situations perceived as threatening with elevations in State Anxiety intensity. State Anxiety is conceptualized as a transitory emotional state or condition of the human organism that is characterized by subjective, consciously perceived feelings of tension and apprehension, and heightened autonomic nervous system activity. Similarity of these ideas to the definition of anxiety given earlier may be noted. One-hour, 20-day, and 104-day test-retest reliabilities for the Trait Anxiety measure range from .73 to .86. This finding on stability is confirmed in a lO-month test by Newmark (1972). Alpha coefficients of internal consistency range from .83 to .92. Correlations of TA with various standard measures of generalized anxiety are high. Significant correlations at the .01 level are reported for Aggression (.44) and Impulsivity (.39); and at the .05 level for Achievement (.2), Endurance (.21), and Affiliation (-.38). A review by Dreger in Euros (1978) states that, in his judgment, the authors of the test provide enough data to show that the trait scores are related to real-life criteria. The review concludes that "The . . . STAI is one of the best of standardized anxiety measures, if not the best." High praise is also to be found in Katkin's review (Buros, 1978), who reveals that there is probably more 124 published research on the STAI than any other anxiety inventory.- This statement is supported by the existence of translations in many languages, and by the large number of references (333) listed by Buros. §g§_(Emotional Well-Being-Physical Symptoms) is a list of physical symptoms that is part of a very comprehensive interview protocol used in the Quality of Employment Survey of the Institute for Social Research (Quinn, et al., 1977). It is included here for its relevance to the EWB variable. No data on reliability or validity are available. Egg (Emotional Well-Being-Check List) is the brief version of the Multiple Affect Adjective Check List (MAACL) developed by Zuckerman and Lubin (1965) to obtain a measure of Generalized Anxiety, Depression, and Hostility. The test manual does not actually contain definitions for these terms. Affect is conceptualized as "the psychological aspects of emotion," presumably to distinguish them from physiological aspects. Test characteristics are satisfactory, though not superior. Median odd-even reliability is reported as .72. Seven-day test-retest reliability ranges from .54 to .7. The three scales of Anxiety, Depression and Hostility are highly intercorrelated. For the present study, this fact is not a problem, since the three are not conceptualized as being independent. The Anxiety and Depression subscales seem to exhibit greater reliability and higher correlations with ratings of these emotions by independent experts. The authors hypothesize that, while the entire test is subject 125 to ”denial defenses," Hostility may be considered especially socially undesirable. Correlations between the Anxiety scale and social desirability response set measures were significant but "not remarkably high," according to the authors. They range mostly from .26 to .48. Similar results and a similar argument led the authors to claim that little of the variance of the MAACL can be explained by an acquiescence response set. They conclude that "response sets do not appear to be an important influence." Despite this barely satisfactory report, review of the test in Euros (1972) is quite favorable. Kelly states that the scales appear to have sufficient differential validity to reflect meaningful differences in affect for groups of subjects. Note that this is exactly the use to which the test is being put in this study. Megargee concludes that the data suggest that MAACL Anxiety scale provides a brief, reasonably valid self- report trait measure of a negative affect condition. Buros (1978) lists 246 references, indicating extensive use. Among these references is the Miller and Seligman (1975) study referred to earlier, and Houston (1972), who suggests that Anxiety was greater when the subjects thought they were helpless to avoid shock. Finally, the ease and rapidity of administration and scoring may be mentioned in favor of MAACL. The scales for the various variables are listed together in Table 4. 126 Aumflq xomno “mcfimm (Hams HmsofluoEmv 40m .v AmEoumexm anaemia " 95mm (Ham: HMdOwHOEmv mam .m , muwmccoflumwsa IdOflbMNflsmmuo Axumwxcm venue. Aauwawnmum usoaonQEmummwev mme .v Immosflumafiumv BHGmB .N 00m .N Amumn omnmflansmnmmwee 0&9 .m “coaumameou Acoflumameou mosmucmm mosmusmm “05.6mm AshamGCOwumwsOImmwg GEES. . m Isoflumecmmuo (Hamz HMGOAUOEHV Immmsflumaeumv 0mm .H Asmfl>umusHamm>Bv BZHB .H mom .H Acofiumuflsmmuo may spas awanOwumamm Amsflwmuaamz HMCOMHoEmv m . HmsofifiocH mo mmmswumeflumv mzm mews omHm mmqmfiHm4> mmbmflmz OB QmmD mflddvm o mqmée 127 Demographic Information Because of the nature of the information solicited in the survey, it was considered inadvisable to request any information that could be viewed as personal identification. The hypotheses do not require such information, but some information about the group as a whole was needed to describe the characteristics of the sample. A demOgraphic information form was consequently included with the survey, but was not stapled to it. This form requested general information about the age, sex, and other such characteristics of the respondents. Data Collection Data were collected from each company in a single visit to the plant. Contact was established in each of the companies, either with the president, plant manager, or personnel manager. Most contacts requested a copy of the questionnaire prior to consenting to participate in the study. This was provided with a request that it not be shown to potential respondents. In most companies, the following procedure was followed: the surveys were given to one of the top officers for distribution to the middle management group, which had been previously identified by the contact person. These surveys were completed during the time members of the top manage- ment group were being interviewed by the writer. They were usually collected by the same person who distributed them. 128 The writer was unable to administer the survey to the respondents in a group setting, which would have been the preferred procedure. CHAPTER VII DATA ANALYSIS RESULTS AND DISCUSSION This chapter describes the procedures that were employed. to prepare the data for hypothesis testing, including missing data manipulations, internal consistency or reliability analysis for the scales in the questionnaire, and discriminant-convergent validity analyses. The results of the analyses that provide information about the hypotheses are then reported and discussed. Missing Data Sixty-nine completed questionnaires were obtained from members of middle management groups of the eight companies in the sample. These contained an average of 4.5 percent missing data. Missing data were concentrated in the sentence-completion portions of the survey. TheY were further concentrated by case; that is, respondents who omitted any sentence completion item tended to omit all. The worst of these cases were removed from the data set. Removing five such cases (7 percent of 69) resulted in a 65 percent reduction in missing data. Of the remaining missing data, 97 percent were located in the sentence completion items. For all the remaining 129 130 missing data, the following procedure was followed: each occurrence of a missing value was substituted by the mean score obtained by the whole group (n = 64) for that particular item. Internal Consistency Coefficient alpha (Cronbach, 1951) was computed from standardized item scores for each of the scales contained in the survey. Note that the sentence completion scales for Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) were coded by two raters each. The ratings are treated as independent scales in this analysis, and the standardized item alpha (Cronbach's alpha using standardized items) is reported for each under the headings P081, P082, ESCl, and ESC2. Table 5 contains internal consistency coefficients for all the scales. The coefficients range from .64 to .85. Examination of item-total correlations for the items in the PIRO scales, P051 and P08 2, revealed that, for 2232 raters, the second item in the scale had a negative correlation with the rest of the scale. This suggested that there might be some problem with the item itself, and it was thus decided to remove the item from subsequent analysis. The standardized item alpha for P051 and P082 with the remaining 14 items in each scale was .71 and .67, respectively. 131 TABLE 5 INTERNAL CONSISTENCY COEFFICIENTS - STANDARDIZED ITEM ALPHA (N = 64) Number of Standardized Scale Items Item Alpha PRIMARINESS OF INDIVIDUAL'S RELATIONf SHIP WITH THE ORGANIZATION POSl: Primariness-Sentence Completion, Rater 1 15 .69 P082: Primariness-Sentence Completion, Rater 2 15 .64 P00: Primariness-Organization Questionnaire 17 .85 TYPE TYPEQ: TYPE-Questionnaire 14 .77 EMOTIONAL WELL-BEING ESCl: Emotional Well-Being, Sentence Completion, Rater l 15 .77 ESCZ: Emotional Well-Being, ' Sentence Completion Rater 2 15 .69 TRAIT: Trait Anxiety 20 .84 EPS: Emotional Well-Being, Physical Symptoms 13 .72 Inter-rater reliabilities were obtained for POS and ESC by computing correlations between the two sets of ratings of each scale produced by the two raters. Inter-rater reliability for POS stood at .86 and for ESC at .84. These reliabilities were considered adequate to justify combining the scores produced by the two raters into one score for each case. 132 Table 6 reports reliability coefficients for POS and ESC after this is done. In addition to coefficient alpha, equal-length Spearman-Brown and Guttman split-half coefficients are also reported.4 The coefficient alpha values for the combined, 28-item POS scale is .84, and for the 30-item ESC scale it is .86. All subsequent analyses involving POS and ESC were performed using this combined scale. TABLE 6 RELIABILITY COEFFICIENTS, COMBINED RATINGS, SENTENCE COMPLETION SCALES (N = 64) Number Guttman of Coeff. Spearman Split— Scale Items Alpha Brown Half POS: Primariness: Sentence . Completion 28 .84 .92 .92 ESC: Emotional Well- Being: Sentence Completion 30 .86 .91 .89 The magnitudes of the internal consistency coefficients were considered sufficiently large to justify combining scores on each item with a scale into one scale score per 4Matzek (1978) states that the Spearman-Brown coeffici- ent was designed to answer the questions: given two equally reliable parts, how reliable would the whole be if they were combined into one? The Guttman split-half coefficient is similar to the Spearman-Brown but does not presume equal reliabilities or equal variances for the two parts. 133 respondent, for each scale in the survey. Subsequent analysis was performed on scale scores, not on item scores . Multi-Trait Multi-Method Matrix The procedures up to this point may be viewed as having the objective of obtaining evidence for the claim that each of the scales employed is sampling from a specific, bounded domain. Evidence for the further claim that some of these. scales are sampling from the game Specific, bounded domain, while others are sampling from different domains,is contained in the multi-trait multivmethod matrix (Campbell and Fiske, 1959): the procedure is more simply called triangulation by Jick (1979). If the scales are grouped by the variables they purport to measure, correlations between scales within a domain should be substantial and larger than correlations between scales across domains. The extent of contribution of method, as compared with the trait, to the correlations obtained can also be examined. Table 7 contains the multi-trait multi-method matrix for data generated by the present survey. Correlations are Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients. Note that the survey contained only one scale for the TYPE variable. Other independent measures for this variable were obtained, but they are all company-level measures and will appear in later discussion. Two scales, POS and POQ, attempt to measure the variable PIRO (primariness of individual's relationship with 134 we. we. ev. Hm. mv. mm. umflq x0030 . .mcfimmlaamz HmcofluOEm "now we. Hm. so.x ma. mH.- maouQESm Hmonmsnm .mcfimmlaamz Hmsofiuoem "mam cm. «H. mm. mo. mumflxae pawns ueHame om. hm. pH. :ofiumamfiou mucousmm a mcwmmlaamz Hmcoauosm "0mm wszmqumz Hmonaozm mm. mm. muflmssowummSOImmwa “0mmwe mews mm. mHHMGsOAummso :oHuMNwsmmuolmmmcflumeum “00m :oflumameoo mocmucmmlmmmsfiumeum “mom onsdezmwmo was mBHz mHmmonemqmm m.qmaoH>HozH mo mmmszmszm "omHm 40m mam BHame 0mm 0mm»? com mom mzm meme omHm Aqm u 21 meeez oomemzuHeqoz eHmmEIHeqaz h mamde 135 the organization). The correlation between these two scales, .66, is the largest correlation in the matrix, suggesting that indeed the two scales do sample from the same domain. Recall that the internal consistency coefficients for POS and POQ were .84 and .85. The two scales employ widely different methods (sentence-completion and Likert-type responses on a five-point scale); thus, a large part of the common variance in these two scales can be attributed to the trait itself. These observations suggested that it would be reasonable to combine POS and POQ into a single scale. This was done and the combined scale is called PIRO (primariness of individual's relationship with the organization). The alpha coefficient for PIRO is .90. The inter-correlations between scales measuring Emotional well-Being are all "substantial" and "larger" than correlations between these scales and scales measuring PIRO and TYPE, with one exception. While the .31 correlation between EPS (EWB-Physical Symptoms) and ESC (EWB-Sentence Completion) is larger than correlations between EPS and all non-Emotional Well-Being scales, it is considerably lower than inter-correlations between other Emotional Well-Being scales. EPS is a list of physical symptoms requiring responses along a four-point Likert-type scale (see page 124). Instructions request the respondent to indicate how often each has happened in the last year. Since the other Emotional Well—Being scales are specifically directed toward the measurement of generalized emotional ill—being, the focus 136 of EPS on recent experience of the respondent might explain its low correlations with the other Emotional Well-Being scales. Since dropping EPS would not cripple the ability of the present study to make reasonably valid tests of the hypotheses, it is not used in subsequent analysis.“ The other three scales for Emotional Well-Being are combined into one score for each respondent, called EWB. The alpha coefficient for EWB is .91. Of especial interest in this analysis is the magnitude of the correlation between POS (Primariness, Sentence Completion) and ESC (Emotional Well-Being, Sentence Completion). A large correlation between these two scales, which have the sentence-completion methodology in common, would have thrown considerable doubt on the ability of the scales to measure the variables in question. The relatively low correlation (.16) suggests that the method itself is not contributing substantially to the variance in the scores on these scales. Organizational Level Analysis The manipulations up to this point have resulted in reducing the data to three scores for each of the 64 respondents to the survey. All except one of the hypotheses of this study are stated at the organizational level. The next step was to aggregate the scores of respondents within each company into one company score for each variable. The three scales that measured TYPE at the organizational level from the outset (TYPE-Interview: TINT; TYPE-Published 137 Data: TPD; TYPE-Employment Stability: _TES) are introduced at this point in the analysis. The final step in preparing the data for hypothesis testing consisted of examining the inter-correlations between the various measures of TYPE, and reducing them to one index, if possible. Interviews from which TINT (TYPE-Interview) scores were obtained were all rated by the writer. Of the total of 19 interviews conducted, a random sample of three interviews was also rated by an independent rater, using the same set of instructions and definitions used by the writer. Each rater produced four judgments for each company, one for each dimension of TYPE, and a global rating of TYPE. Thus, 12 judgments were available from both raters. The Spearman rank order correlation coefficient between these two sets of ratings is .65. This level of correlation has less than .01 probability of chance occurrence (S < .01), suggesting that the ratings are based on information about the focal concept as revealed in the interviews, and are not random. The high correlations between this and other measures of TYPE presented in Table 8 provide further justification for the retention of TINT. An observation made by the independent rater of the interviews deserves special mention. There seemed to be a considerable we-they mentality in the thinking of the top officers of many of the companies, such that there are, in reality, two "TYPES" of organizations co-existing side-by- side, one for management and one for workers. For example, 138 ' there was typically two sets of employment policies in each company, such that the ratings of the company on the dimension of, say, employment stability would be different depending on whether reference was being made to managers or to workers. A company rated as TYPE 2 on the basis of employ- ment stability in the managerial ranks might well turn out to be much closer to TYPE A if employment stability in the worker ranks is computed. TABLE 8 CORRELATION MATRIX, "TYPE" SCALES (N = 8) TYPEQ T INT TPD TES TYPEQ: TYPE-Questionnaire TINT: TYPE-Interview .62 TPD: TYPE-Published Data .32 .63 TES: TYPE-Employment Stability .82 .70 .28 Table 8 lists the inter-correlations between the four scales measuring TYPE: TYPEQ, TINT, TPD, and TES. Note especially that N for this and all subsequent analyses is the number of companies in the sample, eight. Note also that no value of TINT (TYPE-Interview) for company number two was available since it was not possible to conduct interviews with officers from this company. The company was assigned a 139 value of 2.5 for TINT, which was the mean value obtained by the other seven companies on this scale. . Figure 2 summarizes information about the "structure" of the various scales retained for hypothesis testing, and their relationships to each other. FIGURE 2 MEASUREMENT STRUCTURE P051 P082 a=.69 a=.64 (.86) L (.82) l I 1 PCS POQ TYPEQ TINT TES a=.84 a=.85 a=.77 (.66) (.62) (.70)! l f T ' PIRO TYPE a=.90 SB=.90 ESCl ESCZ a=.77 a=.69 (.34) (.44) y F —l ESC TRAIT ECL a=.86 a=.84 I l r EWB a=.91 ‘Numbers in parentheses are correlation coefficients; a is standardized item alpha (Cronbach's alpha with standardized items); SB is an estimate of reliability produced by the Spearman-Brown prophecy formula. 140 Results The results of hypothesis-testing are presented below. First, however, the hypotheses are reproduced here: g1: The correlation between the average level of Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the organization (PIRO) of individuals within an organization and the TYPE of organization will be positive and significantly different from zero. H2: The correlation between the average level of Primariness of Individual's Relationship withthe Organization (PIRO) of individuals within an organization and the average level of Emotional Well-Being (EWB) of individuals within the organization will be positive and significantly different from zero. H3: The correlation between TYPE of organiza- tion and the average level of Emotional Well- Being (EWB) of individuals within the organization will be positive and significantly different from zero. H4: The correlation between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) for all individuals will be positive and significantly different from zero. UDhe null hypothesis in all cases is that of a correlation riot different from zero. 141 Table 9 presents evidence relevant to Hl-3. Pearson product-moment”cOrrelation coefficients and associated. levels of significance are reproduced. One-tailed tests of significance are applied, since the hypotheses are stated in directional terms. TABLE 9 CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS, PIRO TYPE, AND EWB (N = 8) PIRO TYPE EWB PIRO: Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization TYPE .30 (.20) EWB: Emotional Well-Being .72 .33 (.02) (.20) Numbers in parentheses are one-tailed significance levels. H1: While the correlation between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and TYPE of .30 is in the expected direction and not insubstantial, there is nonetheless a 20 percent probability of rejecting a true null hypothesis; that is, a 20 percent probability that the true correlation between PIRO and TYPE is zero, and the observed correlation is merely obtained by chance. H2: The hypothesis pertaining to the relationship between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the 142 Organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) fares much better with a correlation of .72, that is, with 52 percent of the variance in EWB "accounted for" by variance in PIRO. This level of correlation has only a 2 percent probability of chance occurrence when the null hypothesis is actually true; in other words, one may be quite confident in rejecting the null hypothesis of zero correlation. H3: The hypothesis pertaining to the relationship between TYPE and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) fares the same as H1, with an almost identical correlation of .33. This level of correlation has a 20 percent probability of chance occurrence. Table 10 presents the results of an identical analysis in which TYPE (the sum of TYPE-questionnaire, TYPE-Interview, and TYPE-Employment Stability) is replaced by TYPE- Employment Stability (TES). Recall that TES is simply the average length of service of the reSpondents for each company (see page 122). As can be seen from Table 10, if TES had been used as the only indicator of TYPE, both H1 and H3 would have been supported at substantially higher levels of significance. However, TES may not be a very satisfactory measure of TYPE, since it does not incorporate the influence of potential length of service. Information about two factors that affect potential length of service, the age of respondents and the number of years the company has been in existence, is available in this study. Each of these is separately combined with average length of service 143 of respondents (TES) to yield two indices of employment stability. TABLE 10 CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS, PIRO TES, AND EWB (N = 8) PIRO TES EWB PIRO: Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization TES: TYPE-Employment Stability .54 (.08) EWB: Emotional Well-Being .72 .57 (.02) (.07) Numbers in parentheses are one-tailed significance levels. Table 11 presents the Spearman correlation coefficients of these indices with Primariness of Individual's Relation- ship with the organization (PIRO) and with Emotional Well- Being (EWB). When modified by the average age of respondents, TES fails to correlate significantly with PIRO or with EWB. When modified by the number of years the company has been in existence, TES correlates .48 (not significant) with PIRO and .77 (S < .01) with EWB. These results are further discussed in the following section. H4: Evidence relevant to H4 is available from an analysis performed at the individual level of analysis. The correlation between Primariness of Individual's’ Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and Emotional 144 Well-Being (EWB) is .36 (N = 64), which is found to be significant at the .002 level. One may be quite confident in rejecting the null hypothesis of zero correlation between PIRO and EWB at this individual level of analysis (which disregards Specific organizational membership). TABLE 11 CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS, TWO INDICES OF EMPLOYMENT STABILITY WITH PIRO AND EWB ESl* ESZ** PIRO: Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the .36 .48 Organization (.19) (.11) EWB: Emotional Well-Being .29 .77 (.25) (.01) *ESl = TBS/Average Age of ReSpondent **ESZ = TBS/(Age of Company/Age of Youngest Company) Numbers in parentheses are one-tailed significance levels. Table 9-A presents the results of applying an attenuation correction formula to the correlations reported in Table 9. The objective of this analysis is to examine what the magnitude of the correlations would be if the scales measuring the variables were perfectly reliable. Cronbach's alpha coefficients of internal consistency for the composite measures of PIRO and EWB stood at .90 and .91, respectively. Reliability of the TYPE composite measure was estimated as 145 .90 by the Spearman-Brown Prophecy formula. TES was assumed to have a reliability of 1.00; while self-reports of date of joining an organization might be expected to suffer from some unreliability, the procedure used here would lead to a more conservative estimate of corrected correlation. TABLE 9-A .CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS CORRECTED FOR ATTENUATION, PIRO, TYPE AND EWB (N = 8) PIRO TYPE EWB PIRO: Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization TYPE . 3 3 (.20) EWB: Emotional Well-Being .80 .37 (.01) (.18) Numbers in parentheses are one-tailed significance levels. The correlations in Table 9—A may be viewed as estimates of the "true" correlation between the variables. Because of the fairly high estimates of internal consistency, the corrected correlations are not much higher than the uncorrected ones. The shifts in the level of significance are not substantial either. The discussion of the results below, which uses uncorrected correlations,wou1d not be substantially different if corrected correlations were used. 146 Discussion Hypothesis 1 Nine percent of the variation in each variable in the Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO)-TYPE pair of variables is associated with variation in the other variable in the pair. In the murky world of the social sciences, even the faint glimmer ofaarelationship is not to be discarded lightly, especially in view of the small size of the sample. The main shortcoming of the study might then simply be the small sample size. Another possible explanation of the absence of a stronger obtained relationship between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and TYPE is that either or both of the variables might suffer from inadequate measurement. Inadequate measurement might result from lack of conceptual clarity in the descriptions of the variables, or from failure to construct precise measurement instruments. With respect to the latter possibility, the convergent and discriminant validity information presented in Tables 7 and 8 suggest that the main problem is not one of the precision of the measurement instruments. Furthermore, the independence of much of the information used for TYPE from that used for PIRO gives further confidence in the validity of the measures. Of the two variables, Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization can probably be rated 147 higher on the "conceptual clarity" dimension than TYPE, not only because TYPE is "borrowed" from external sources (Ouchi and Johnson, 1978), but also because TYPE is a compound of three concepts (Wholistic Concern, Employment Stability, and Global PhilOSOphy of Management). Recall also that the original seven dimensions of TYPE were reduced to three in this study; it is possible that three are too few or that they are not the "correct" ones to adequately represent the original concept. Alternatively, three may be two too many. Recall that a single index of Employment Stability, TES, is able to "account for" 29 percent of the variance in PIRO. While the significance of this remark may be tempered somewhat by the results presented in Table 11, one may recall also that TYPE has its origins in Japanese management methods, in which Employment Stability is a strong and central feature. It has earlier been suggested (page 80) that TYPE Z characteristics represent an organizational environment in which employees are able to form a primary relationship with the organization. It is not unreasonable to suppose that it is simply the assurance of (relatively) permanent employment that enables the employee to make the emotional investment characteristic of a primary relationship. Still another possible explanation for the absence of a stronger obtained relationship between TYPE and PIRO is that none of the three dimensions (Employment Stability, Wholistic Concern, Global PhilOSOphy of Management) is a 148 characteristic that encourages Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization. Four other possibilities may be raised in this context: 1. Since the scales TYPE-Questionnaire (TYPEQ) and TYPE-Interview (TINT) both measure at no better than the interval level of measurement, it is possible that the companies in the sample are concentrated at one end of the TYPE A-TYPE z continuum. In other words, employees rating their company on this variable would use as end points of the dimension extremes that they can conceptualize based on their own experience. If they know of no company with better employment stability than their own company, they would tend to rate their own company ”very high” on this dimension, even if actual employment stability may be low in absolute terms. This restriction in range would naturally result in low correlations between TYPE and other variables. Furthermore, the relative positions of the companies in the sample on TYPE would also be subject to greater error, since the difference between "high" and "very high" would be less distinct for a respondent who has experienced a smaller range of values on the dimension in question than one who has experienced a larger range. Some evidence that this phenomenon may be operating here can be found upon re-examination of the interview data; for example, none of the companies in the sample demonstrated any substantial degree of "global philOSOphy of management," 149 and all of them reported more or less substantial lay-offs at the time of interview. 2. The depressed conditions in the industry may have influenced respondent scores on TYPE, especially on TYPE- Questionnaire (TYPEQ). The direction of this influence is unpredictable since some of the respondents might feel gratitude and relief that their company has not yet released them,and others might feel insecure and distrustful of the company. 3. There is evidence in the interview data, as mentioned earlier (page 137), that it may be inappropriate to think of companies as having a single value on TYPE. One gets the rather strong impression that the companies in the sample view rank-and-file employees considerably differently than they view white-collar employees. Especially, it seems clear that many of the companies in the sample view rank-and- file as "they," to be dealt with strictly according to provisions of the "contract." In other words, companies seek to be one "type" of organization to white-collar employees, and another "type" to rank-and-file. All the respondents to the survey were from the white-collar category, but the questions in the survey do not discriminate between the two "types" of policies. For example, the response to the item, "the company avoids laying off employees" can range from "very true" to "not at all true," depending upon which group of employees the respondent is 150 thinking of in his/her response. This would tend to seriously undermine the TYPE measures. 4. Finally, shortcomings in the measurement of one of the indices of TYPE, TES, were mentioned before (page 138). Is the average length of service of respondents in company A comparable to the average length of service of respondents in company B if the average ages of respondents in company A and company Bare 50 years and 25 years, respectively? Similar doubts may be raised about the comparability of average length of service of respondents across Campanies with varying dates of establishment. Table 11 (page144) presents correlations of length of service (TES) modified by age of respondents and age of company with rimariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization and with Emotional Well-Being. The mixed results again suggest that re-examination of the TYPE variable is required. For example, recent growth in a company which resulted in a large addition to the work force would reduce the average length of service of the employees. The employment stability indices used here would all be lowered. But clearly, recent growth is not to be taken as any evidence of a movement of the company towards TYPE A. Hypothesis 2 If faint glimmers of relationship in the murky waters of the social sciences are not to be discarded lightly, neither should findings of extremely strong relationship be spared careful scrutiny. While it is hoped that the .72 151 correlation between Primariness of Individual's Relationship R with the organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) is valid, and hence indicative of a carefully worked out and valid theory, some sources of possible spuriousness may be mentioned. All the information used in producing this correlation coefficient came from the same source--the respondents to the survey. Thus, the most likely source of common variance, other than the relationship hypothesized, is simply the lack of independence of sources of information for PIRO and EWB. To the extent that there is evidence of responses having been obtained from different domains of experience within the individual (discriminant validity), the suspicion that high correlation is an artifact of the lack of independence of variable values can be discounted. Examination of the multi-trait, multiemethod matrix (Table 7) suggests that lack of independence might be a more significant problem for the POQ (Primariness-Questionnaire) measure of PIRO than for the POS (Primariness-Sentence Completion) measure, since POQ has moderately substantial correlations with all three of the scales used in emotional well-being (.37, .32, and .42) even before the data is aggregated by company-membership. Before examining the correlation between primariness-sentence completion (POS) and EWB, common-method between POS and ESC (emotional well- being: sentence completion, one of the components of EWB) may also be suggested as another source of spuriousness in 152 the PIRO-EWB relationship. These observations led to the computation of the correlation coefficient, at the organiza- tional level of analysis, between POS alone and EWB with ESC removed from it. Surprisingly, the correlation between these two stood at .73, identical to the correlation between PIRO and EWB. Possible conceptual proximity of the variables PIRO and EWB might also be considered in this context, even though examination of the definitions and descriptions of the two variables (see page 96): does not provide much support for this suggestion. Empirically as well, Table 7 (page 134) contains evidence for the claim of success in discriminating between the two variables. Hypothesis 3 Comments made under Hl above apply equally to results of the hypothesis test for H3. The correlation between TYPE and Emotional Well-Being is .33, accounting for about 11 percent of the variance in the variables. While this is not insubstantial, it has a 20 percent probability of chance occurrence when the null hypothesis of zero correlation is true . Note than, when TYPE-Employment Stability (TES) alone is used as a measure of TYPE, the correlation between it and Emotional Well-Being stands at .57, which is significant at the .07 level. If substituting TES for TYPE results in substantial increases in its correlations with both EWB and PIRO, this further suggests that the "problem" is to be 153 located in TYPE and not in EWB and/or PIRO. The use of two standard scales in Emotional Well-Being also suggests the same thing. Possible shortcomings with the concept and measurement of TYPE have been discussed earlier (page 148). Note that H3 is similar conceptually to Ouchi and Johnson's (1978) main result, though considerably different operationally. Only three of their seven dimensions of TYPE were used in this study, and few of the measures of TYPE or Emotional Well-Being that Ouchi and Johnson used were employed here. Furthermore, the analysis employed here is considerably different. Consequently, results obtained here can be no more than suggestive in terms of their implications for Ouchi and Johnson's work. One may say that the results obtained here provide "limited support" for Ouchi and Johnson's results. TYPE, as interpreted in this study, failed to discriminate very sharply between companies that had different scores on Emotional Well-Being but the direction of the relationship obtained is the same as predicted by Ouchi and Johnson, and the magnitude of the relationship is not insubstantial. In reviewing Ouchi and Johnson's (1978) paper, one notices with interest that, of the seven dimensions they used, employment stability was the dimension containing the greatest difference between the TYPE A and TYPE Z organizations. To the extent that the present study contains some evidence of a similar nature, it tends to confirm at least a small part of Ouchi and Johnson's results. 154 Hypothesis 4 With N = 64, there is only one-fifth of 1 percent probability that a correlation of .36 between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) could have been obtained by chance when the true correlation is zero. Nonetheless, it is important to remember that the variance accounted for in this relationship is only 13 percent. PIRO is the one variable which is somewhat unique'to this study, constructed to explore the process by which TYPE of organization might be related to Emotional Well-Being. The results obtained suggest that PIRO is significantly associated with EWB, but that it is not the only "explanation" for high Emotional Well-Being. There is, of course, no suggestion in the ideas discussed in Part I that PIRO was expected to be the only or even major explanation for EWB. There are many sources of primary-relatedness in an individual's life. This study has only sought to suggest that work organization is one source, destined, as discussed in Part I, to become a major source if industrialization is not to be accompanied by alienation and anomie. Possible sources of spuriousness are mentioned above. "Relationship" involves the individual's thoughts and emotions, and a source of common variance between PIRO and EWB might be a "halo" phenomenon, where how one feels in general may influence how one "feels" about or towards the organization, even though the former may be attributed 155 to entirely non-PIRO influences on EWB. Alternatively, how one feels about the organization may influence how one-rates oneself on EWB when there is no "real" relationship. This second possibility is of special concern in this study, since the scales assessing PIRO all preceded physically the scales assessing EWB. If the reSpondent found himself "feeling pleased" about the organization while he was responding to the PIRO items, this might result in his feeling reluctant to "complain" about how he generally feels, in the EWB section of the survey. This would inflate the present result, leading to an erroneous conclusion that a relationship exists between PIRO and EWB. Possible explanations for the lack of a larger correlation need also to be mentioned. The most serious source of contamination is probably social desirability or defensive reSponse sets stimulated by both PIRO and EWB items. The items are fairly transparent with reSpect to their intent. Furthermore, the surveys were not administered by, or in the presence of, the writer, so it is not known what might have been said to the respondents before they filled out the survey. The placement of the EWB scales at the end of the survey was designed to control for defensive response set, as was the request that the questionnaire be administered by the writer to the respondents as a group. This request was denied by every company in the sample on the grounds that it would be too disruptive. 156 In conclusion, the least that can be said about the PIRO-EWB results at individual level of analysis is that they provide enough reason to pursue exploration and refine- ment of these ideas. Summary of Results All the correlations are in the expected direction, and none is less than .30. Correlations between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) are significant, both at the individual level of analysis and at the organizational level of analysis. Correlations of TYPE with PIRO and with EWB are not significant. The "weakest" variable in the study seems to be TYPE. A large part of this weakness probably has conceptual sources, both in terms of the elaboration of the concept itself and in terms of the specification of its relationship with the other variables. There is some evidence to suggest that employment stability may be a central dimension in the TYPE construct. There is considerable room for improvement in conceptual as well as methodological areas. There is considerable justification for pursuing this line of inquiry further. CHAPTER VIII IMPLICATIONS AND CONCLUSIONS In this chapter, the implications of the research reported in the last three chapters are discussed. The particulars of the research effbrt impose certain limitations on the kinds of conclusions that may be made. Thus, the first task of this chapter is to discuss briefly these limitations. The research reported here was viewed as a lesson in ”disciplined inquiry" (Cronbach and Suppes, 1969) and as a construct validation exercise. The results reported in Chapter VII may thus be viewed from each of these perspectives, as a measure of the success of the learning experience and as a measure of the validity of the conceptual- izations on which the research was based. Both of these perspectives are employed below. Within each perspective, the discussion deals with internal consistency/reliability analyses and with the correlational analyses. Implications for future research are suggested throughout the following discussion. The most significant of these are collected together in the final section of the chapter, along with other major conclusions of the research. 157 158 Limitations on Interpretation Generalizability Limited genralizability may be the most important restriction to be placed on the conclusions to be drawn from this research. There is no reason to believe that the sample from which the data were obtained is representative of the pOpulation of "U.S. work organizations" implied in the theoretical position developed in Chapter IV. The procedures by which participation of the organizations in the sample were obtained are replete with selectivity of various kinds. Use only of companies engaged in the manufacture of automobile parts, for example, might impose a severe restriction of range on the TYPE of organization included in the sample. More than 30 companies in the industry were contacted with requests for cooperation in the study,of whom only eight consented. Again, this self-selection imposes serious limitations on generalizability. Similarly, the sample cannot be assumed to be representative of the population to which the theoretical propositions make reference along a wide variety of dimensions, including size, age, profit- ability, nature and location of business, nature of ownership, and so on. This restriction of generalizability was expected right from the design stage of the project. That is, it was at least partially deliberately imposed to accommodate limitations of time and resources, as well as limitations of the sc0pe of the study, in that a more comprehensive 159 project would have been inappropriate at this stage even if it had been possible. Along with this restriction on "spatial" generalizability may be mentioned a restriction on "temporal" generalizability, which may be specially relevant for the present research. The economically depressed conditions in the industry from which the sample was drawn may be expected to influence the variables of the study in various, unpredictable ways, such that the results obtained here may not be replicable, even with the same sample and the same respondents, a few months from now. This restriction of generalizability along the temporal dimensions is, of course, an unplanned and unfortunate consequence of the present atypical situation in the industry. In a more technical vein, limitations on the interpretation of correlational analysis have the same consequence of restricting generalizability. While the use of correlations and regressions as descriptive statistics does not require any assumptions about "the form of the distribution, the variability of y scores within x columns, or the true level of measurement represented by the scores" (Hays, 1973, page 636), assumptions do become necessary in the use of correlations to make inferences about true linear relationships in populations. The same point is made by Carroll, quoted by Glass and Stanley (1970): No assumptions are necessary for the computation of a Pearsonian coefficient, but the interpretation of its meaning certainly depends upon the extent to which the data conform to an appropriate statistical 160 model for making this interpretation. As actual data depart from a fit to such a model, the limits of the correlation coefficient may contract, and the adjec- tival interpretations are less meaningful (page 126) . In summary, the results of the research reported here can- not be used to make any strong conclusions about the theoretical propositions described in the first part of this dissertation, since the theoretical statements are naturally meant to have general and broad applicability. This was not unexpected: it is for this reason that the research was described in Chapter IV as "exploratory" and as a "construct validation exercise" (see page 90). Evidence from a large number of studies in various settings with random selection of organizations for inclusion in the sample, is required to achieve generalizability. Interpretations of Correlations Other limitations on the interpretation of correlational analysis may also be mentioned. The most well-known of these is that correlations calculated on the basis of non- experimentally obtained data provide information only about association or co-variation, not about causality. The tendency to view the .72 correlation between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) as evidence that Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization causes Emotional Well-Being must be carefully avoided. This tendency exists partly because such a causal relationship is implied and even stated in the theoretical position 161 developed in the first part of the dissertation.5 Simple correlational analysis based on one-time survey data contains no evidence relevant to the possibilities that it might be Emotional Well-Being that "causes" Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization, or that a third variable (for example, primariness orientation) may be the cause for both. Closely related to this are limitations imposed by what Cronbach and Suppes (1969) called the indivisibility of social systems: "The essential difficulty in research on social processes is the interaction of variables" (page 141). Thus, while employment stability seems the most promising of the organizational dimensions as an explanation of a primary relation between the employee and the organization, one suspects that employment stability is one of several factors--organizational, environmental, and individual-- that come together to result in primariness of the individual- organization relationship. One notes, for example, the wide cultural context within which the ngnkg, or permanent employment system, of Japanese industry exists. As Hodgson (1978) former U.S. Secretary of Labor and former ambassador to Japan, observed: American society is first and foremost underpinned by the venerable Judeo-Christian objective of individual justice. The Japanese, however, spurn individual justice as a priority goal. Instead, they seek something in many ways the opposite; they seek group harmony. We American 5An examination of Chapter VII reveals that discussion of this pair of variables always lists PIRO first and EWB second, evidence that the tendency does exist. 162 justice-seekers speak proudly of our rights. The harmony-minded Japanese stress not rights but relations. . . . In American life the individual- strives to stand out. The Japanese citizen, however, seeks to fit in, and fit in he does-~into his family, gis schooIT—hIE company, his union, his nation (page That environmental, individual and organizational variables other than TYPE are not considered in this study is, of course, a limitation of the theoretical framework, but the point being made here is that simple correlational analysis does not reveal information about interactions. Tasks for the future suggested by these considerations are incorporation of anthropological, sociological, psychological, and organizational variables other than TYPE in the explana- tion for primariness of the individual-organization relation- ship, and the use of more SOphisticated statistical techniques to test relationships among these variables. Two other limitations on the interpretation of correlations may be mentioned. (1) The possibility exists that a substantial correlation may be due to the presence of "identifiable groups of subjects with different means" (Glass and Stanley, 1970, page 123). If organizations located in rural areas tend to have higher scores on Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization and Emotional Well Being than organizations located in urban areas, pooling these groups together may produce a high correlation between the two, even though within each group the variables correlate essentially zero. (2) The existence of non-linear relationships would not be revealed at all by 163 a. linear correlational anaylsis. While the possibility of non- linear relationships can be checked by the use of scatterplots, the small number of companies in the present sample limits the use of scatterplots for this purpose. Alow correlation between TYPE and Emotional Well-Being (EWB) may be due to the reasons suggested in Chapter VII, or it may mask a high non-linear relationship: it may be that "pure" TYPE A and TYPE Z companies, by being obviously one or the other, attract employees temperamentally suited to working in them; while an organization intermediate between TYPE A and TYPE Z may not be able to attract the right type of employees because of the mixed signals it provides. Linear correlational analysis would not reveal the non-linear relationship between TYPE and Emotional Well-Being that might result from this situation. Conclusion All these limitations are discussed here to justify not drawing any strong conclusions about the theoretical prOpositions, either confirmatory or dis-confirmatory, from the results of this exploratory study. The low correlations of TYPE with Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) and with Emotional Well-Being (EWB) does not invalidate the suggestion thataawork organiza- tion can create an environment which encourages formation of a primary relationship between it and its employees. Nor does the high correlation between Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization and Emotional Well-Being justify the conclusion that individuals need 164 primary relations to achieve and maintain psychological equilibrium. This does not mean, of course, that nothing useful came out of the experiences reported in this study, as the following discussion should make abundantly clear. Lessons inDisciplined Inquiry Scientific research may be viewed as the process by which figments <16 one's imagination are transformed into conceptual tools for communication and for the achievement of human purposes. The process consists of subjecting the products of one's imagination to the dual disciplines of logic and data: not only must one's ideas and conceptualiza- tions be internally consistent and loqically sound, they must also be demonstrably grounded in reality. While intuition and bias must necessarily inform imagination throughout the researcher's career, ideas and images remain in the realm of poetry until they have been subjected to logical and empirical scrutiny. Beginning with vaguely formulated ideas, the researcher confronted with the task of obtaining empirical confirmation for the "real world" existence of the phenomenon of interest must first and foremost bridge the gap between his ideas and the experiences of people in the real world. To the extent that the experiences of reSpondents conform to the researcher's conceptualizations of these experiences, as contained in his hypotheses, to that extent responses to a series of stimuli designed by the researcher will conform to statistical patterns suggested in the hypotheses. This 165 requires the stimuli, or questionnaire items, to be grounded in the experiences of prospective reSpondents--in other words, be in fact "stimulating." They must be couched in language the respondent is likely to understand, and must refer to concerns and interests the respondent is likely to have. At the same time, they must emerge entirely from the conceptualizations that are being subjected to empirical test. They must derive directly from the definitions of the variables, and refer to no more and no less than the implications of the definitions. The researcher's success depends on the degree of clarity of conceptualization and specially on his ability to imagine the world from the perspective of the prospective reSpondents. Information about the degree of success actually achieved is contained in the analyses of the data finally collected.) The internal consistency coefficient measures the extent to which responses to items in a scale correlate with each other. For example, the .85 coefficient of internal consistency for the l7-item primariness-organization questionnaire (POQ) scale of Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization indicates a high degree of success in translation of the concept from its vague, original formulation to 17 specific items that tap the same domain of experience in the respondent. The values for this coefficient range from .67 to .85 for the scales for which this coefficient could be computed, indicating reasonable to high degrees of success of operationalization. 166 Of special interest in this context are the internal- consistency coefficients for the sentence-completion scales, in which sources of "noise" include not only inadequate translation of concepts to questionnaire items, but also inadequate communication of concepts to raters. Fair success, represented by the .67 to .77 range of values for internal consistency suggests not only that the items elicited reSponses from a specific, reasonably bounded domain, but also that the domain of interest was adequately communicated to raters. Items had to achieve a balance between being structured and specific enough to tap the domain of interest, yet being unstructured enough to retain the projective nature of the sentence-completion methodology. Two comments may be made in the context of this discussion about the sentence-completion methodlogy. (1) As stated earlier (page 118),this methodology was selected to avoid the problem of "non-attitudes" (Sellitz, et al., 1979). Likert-type questionnaire items may elicit responses from respondents even though they may not agree with any of the alternatives provided, or even though they may not ever haVe thought about the subject of the item. By not limiting the respondent to a specific set of alternatives, sentence-completions, and other projective- type methodologies, avoid or reduce this potential threat to validity. The reduction in reliability that may be the price for this increase in validity was discussed above. (2) A couple of limitations of the methodology may be 167 mentioned. The influence of expressive ability of respondents on the responses they make is a cause for concern, specially if this expressive ability varies systematically with other variables of interest. Future use of this methodology might be designed to control for this influence. Secondly, the "cost" associated with the coding of the responses, in terms of rater time, may limit use of the methodology considerably. Reduction of the number of items seems inadvisable in the face of the range of internal consistency coefficients obtained in this study (.67 to .77). Internal consistency analysis only provides information relevant to the claim that bounded domains of experience of respondents have been tapped. Evidence relevant to the claim that these domains are the ones described by the researcher is mostly contained in discriminant and convergent validity analyses, and is discussed in the next section. Combined with "face validity," internal consistency does provide some information relevant to construct validity, since, if the scale items are all transparently related to the construct as defined, high inter-correlation among responses to items contains evidence not only that a bounded domain has been tapped, but that the specific bounded domain referenced by the concept has been located. In conclusion, the most important lesson to emerge from this experience might be that the gap between "the ideas of the researcher and the experience of people in 168 the real world" (see page 164) can be minimized from the out- set by conceptualizing Operationally, that is, by viewing "conceptualization" and "operationalization" as a unitary task. If concepts were developed right from the beginning with the awareness that they will have to be subjected to the discipline of data, by oneself or by others, the result would be sharper conceptualization and easier construction of reliable measuring instruments. Construct Validity If respondent scores on different scales designed to measure the same variable correlate substantially with each other, one's confidence is strengthened that the phenomenon described by the variable does exist in the real world. This is even more true if the scales employ different methodologies. If reSpondent scores on a scale designed to measure variable A correlates substantially lg§§_with scores on a scale designed to measure variable B, this lends weight to the claim that variable A is distinct from variable B. This is even more true if the scales employ the same methodology. Information on convergent and discriminant validity of this type is contained in the multi-trait multi-method matrix (Table 7, page 134) and in Table 8 (page 138). Finally, the strongest evidence of construct validity is the demonstration that the variable "behaves" in relation to other variables as predicted by theory, that it "fits in" as expected in a nomolOgical network. This study contains 169 all these types of evidence. Each of the three constructs of the study is discussed below. Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization (PIRO) Respondent scores on the two scales employed in this study to measure PIRO (primariness-organization-sentence completion: POS; and primariness-organization-questionnaire: POQ) correlate .66 with each other. Note that both scales had internal consistency coefficients higher than their correlation with each other, a prerequisite for the conclusions being made here. The two scales employ widely different methodologies: sentence-completions and Likert- type questionnaire. These facts contain strong evidence that the scales measure the same thing, hence that the concept as described exists in the experience of the 64 respondents to the survey. The average correlation between POS and five other scales designed to measure other variables in the study has a magnitude of .19, and the correlation between POS and the only other scale employing the same methodology (Emotional Well-Being: sentence completion: ESC) is .16, indicating that Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization as measured by P05 is quite distinct from the other two variables of the study, TYPE and Emotional Well- Being (EWB). In contrast, the average correlation between POQ, the questionnaire measure of PIRO, and five other scales designed to measure other variables is .32, and the average 170 correlation between POQ and three scales employing the same methodology but measuring different variables is .29. Thus, evidence that POQ measures a phenomenon distinct from TYPE and Emotional Well-Being rests on less firm empirical ground than evidence that POS, the sentence- completion measure of PIRO, measures a phenomenon distinct from TYPE and EWB. If:U:were not for the high correlation between POS (the sentence-completion measure of PIRO) and POQ (the Likert-type measure of PIRO), use of POQ in subsequent analyses would have had weak justification. It is possible that some response sets commonly associated with Likert-type scales may be operating in the data generated by POQ. It is also possible that POQ, as well as the other Likert-type scales, may be eliciting "non-attitudes." Referring now to the correlations of the composite measure of Primariness of Individualls Relationship with the Organization with the composite measures of TYPE and of Emotional Well-Being reported in Table 9, significant positive correlations were predicted by theory. Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization and Emotional Well-Being correlate .72 in the data obtained in this study, which is significant at the .02 level. This result lends considerable support to the construct validity of PIRO, especially since there is strong evidence for the validity of the Emotional Well-Being construct (see below). The high internal consistency estimates of the composite measures of both variables (see page 139) may also be cited 171 in favor of this conclusion. Note that limitations on generalizability discussed earlier suggest that this confidence in the primariness-of-individual's-relationship- with-the-organization concept be somewhat tempered by consideration of the possibility that it .existence and operation according to theory may be some artifact of the particular sample employed. In contrast, the .30 correlation between Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization and TYPE is not significant. This suggests that (l) the present operationalization of PIRO is inadequate, (2) the present operationalization of TYPE is inadequate, and/or (3) PIRO and TYPE do not relate with each other as suggested by theory in this sample. Of these possibilities, (l) is the least plausible, in view of the discussions above. Elaboration of the second suggestion is contained in the discussion of TYPE in the next section, which raises some doubts about the concept and its measurement in this study. The third possibility cannot be ruled out, however, even if TYPE does prove.tx> be less than satisfactorily Operationalized in this study. Limited generalizability may again be kept in mind, but the negative result should lead to a close examination of the ideas that resulted in the formulation of the hypothesis of a significant positive relationship between PIRO and TYPE. Some of this examination was begun in Chapter VII. The possibility that Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization may be 172 associated with cultural and social variables was raised earlier (page 161). PIRO may also be associated with. individual variables other than emotional well-being, such as educational achievement, salary, age, and so on. It may also be associated with organizational variables other than TYPE, either with some other organizational Structural variables, or even with variables such as industry or product. Finally, for those would would see PIRO in these terms, the adequacy of applying concepts originating from the Japanese culturetx> organizations in the U.S.A. may also be questioned. In conclusion, while the data justify strong confidence in the concept of Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization, they also suggest close examination of the propositions regarding the determinants of PIRO. Further examination of anthropological, sociological, organizational, and psychological literature is indicated. Future research should include assessment of possible determinants of PIRO in addition to the ones used here, as well as incorporate improvements in the concept and measurement of the TYPE construct, as suggested below. TYPE Only one scale for measuring TYPE was included in the multi-method multi-trait matrix (Table 7, page 134), since only one scale for TYPE was included in the questionnaire administered to respondents. Other measures of TYPE were also obtained, from independent sources, and inter- correlations among the various measures of TYPE are reported 173 in Table 8 (page 138). Convergent validity information available in Table 8 (page 138) strongly suggests that the various measures of TYPE succeed in measuring the same thing. The average inter-correlation among four measures of TYPE, TYPEQ (questionnaire), TINT (interview), TPD (published data) and TES (employment stability) is .56. In contrast, the average correlation of the questionnaire measure of TYPE, TYPEQ, with six scales measuring different variables had a magnitude of .21, while the average correlation of TYPEQ with three scales employing the same methodology was .18. These numbers contain moderate evidence that TYPEQ succeeded in tapping a domain of experience in respondents distinct from Primariness of Individual's Relationship with the Organization and Emotional Well-Being. Note, also, that estimates of reliability are not consistently available for these scales, reducing their interpretability considerably. The correlation of TYPE with Emotional Well-Being (.33) and with Primariness of the Individual's Relationship with the Organization (.30) both fail to achieve the stipulated level of significance. In other words, evidence that the concept measured by the TYPE scales is the same concept as suggested by theory is weak, suggesting limited success of Operationalization. To the extent that the high inter- correlations among various measures of TYPE suggest con- struct validity, the low correlations of TYPE with PIRO and EWB suggest re-examination of the theory as well. 174 At the Operational level, the experiences represented by the numbers above suggest the following: (1) The writer's inexperience with interviewing probably accounted for a paucity of information on some of the dimensions of the TYPE variable. (2) The influence of prior familiarity of some interviewees with the questionnaire, which could not be avoided, serves to shed further doubt on interview data. (3) The reliability of ratings of interview data was not adequately assessed.r Interview data might better have been coded by a rater other than the interviewer, and more than three interviews might have been independently rated. (4) While independent measures of variables are recommended, they must generate data whose reliability can be estimated. For example, while the internal consistency of TYPEQ, the questionnaire measure of TYPE, could be computed, no estimate for the reliability of TYPE-employment stability (TES) was available. Adequate thought to the assessment of reliability was not given prior to data collection. Future research involving use of interviews, and involving assessment of the TYPE variable, should incorporate the improvements suggested above. The experience with the TYPE variable reported here also suggests some conceptual problems. Some possibilities were identified in Chapter VII. With reference to the discussion about conceptualization above, TYPE may be singled out as the variable in which the gap between the researcher's ideas and the experience of 175 "real world" peOple remained the widest. Partly, this might be because the opErationalization of this variable did not involve writing as many structured and semi-structured D items as it did, for example, in the operationalization of primariness of individual's relationship with the organization. Wholistic Concern and Global Philosophy of Management, two dimensions of TYPE about which the interviews revealed little, seem not to have been defined in terms of the experiences of prospective respondents. As conceptualized at present, they remain "slippery." An urgent task for the future is clarification and elaboration of these dimensions. There is confusion, as well, in the idea of employment stability, which is the third dimension cxf TYPE used in this study. As used in the scales of TYPE employed here, employment stability is simply the length of tenure of respondents or employees. But length of tenure must be evaluated in terms of potential length of tenure of respondents in a particular company, which will be influenced by such factors as age of employees and date of establishment of company. Furthermore, how adequate a measure of an organizational policy to encourage employment stability might length of tenure be? TYPE makes reference to the existence of such an organizational policy. Employment stability as measured also