THE INFLUENCE OF INCREASED DURATIONS OF SPEECH STIMULI ON THE RECOGNITION AND RETENTION OF WORDS? BY APHASIC ADULTS Thesis for “we Deg-m of pit. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY William R. Dopheide 1968 w s! wmg,“;z {may-r... fr LIBRARY ” Michigan Scam Ilaivcnfigy v—‘w 4,) '01 ' This is to certify that the ~ . .‘. v “ s '5, , thesis entitled ’ THE INFLUENCE OF INCREASEb DURATIONS OF SPEECH STIMULI on THE RECOGNITION’AND RETENTION OF wonns BY APHASIC'ADUL$S*“ “ presented By WILLIAM R. DOPh’EIDE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for 3—0.9;— degree in AUDIOLmY AND SPEECH SCIENCES 2a: my my M... m» (Jo‘s-sear” 5" /é " 4 5’ Date 0-169 ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF INCREASED DURATIONS OF SPEECH STIMULI ON THE RECOGNITION AND RETENTION OF WORDS BY APHASIC ADULTS by William R. Dopheide Recognizing the importance of auditory language reception in the total rehabilitative effort directed toward the language problems of the aphasic adult, Speech patholo- gists have sought to develop modifications of the auditory language stimulus that will make the stimulus more adequate for the aphasic adult. One recommended modification that appears to have rather widespread clinical application is increasing the duration of the words spoken to the aphasic adult. The purpose of this investigation was to assess the effectiveness of temporal expansions in the phonemic seg- ments of words on the aphasic adult's performance in recog- nizing and retaining words. In addition, two independent (control) variables were employed: meaningfulness of the stimulus word and subjects' adequacy in auditory language function. The dependent variables were performance on a word recognition task and performance on a retention Span task, with stimuli being presented auditorily on both tasks. These tasks were selected since they seemed highly related William R. Dopheide to significant characteristics of the aphasic language deficit, according to reports in the literature. For the word recognition task, the stimulus words, derived from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, were arranged in a manner that provided four equivalent lists of 75 words each. Each list was divided arbitrarily into three sets of 25 words each that were regarded as reflect- ing three levels of meaningfulness. The stimuli for the retention span task varied in complexity from two word series to five word series, with all complexity levels equally represented at all stimulus durations. The varia— tions from the normal durations of the stimulus words were produced by an electromechanical process to the niminal levels of 125%, 150%, and 175% expansions of the original recorded duration (100%); hence, four stimulus duration conditions were available. Subjects were assigned to a High auditory language disturbance group or a Low auditory language disturbance group on the basis of their scores on the Auditory Disturbance section of the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia. The data were treated by analysis of variance: three factor (repeated measures) for Meaningfulness, Groups, and Durations on the word recognition task; and two factor (repeated measures) for Groups and Durations on the reten- tion Span task. An inter-task comparison was made employing a Spearman rank correlation at each stimulus duration con- dition. William R. DOpheide No significant main effect for Duration was observed on either task; however, Groups diSplayed some interaction with Durations on the word recognition task (but not on the retention task). The interaction appeared to be generated by the High auditory language disturbance group. The Mean- ingfulness interaction with Durations, which was of interest, was not statistically significant. Of importance to the design of the experiment, the main effects for Groups and Meaningfulness levels were statistically significant and manifested no interaction. The inter-task comparison re- vealed statistically significant correlations in the High auditory language disturbance group at three of the four duration conditions. This finding might be interpreted as meaning less independence of available vocabulary and retention Span in aphasic adults with poor auditory lan- guage function. On the basis of this investigation, increased stimulus durations, as produced for use in this study, do not have a general positive influence on word recognition and retention span. Increasing the duration of the stimulus may have a positive effect on word recognition for subjects with poor auditory language function, but further research is necessary. THE INFLUENCE OF INCREASED DURATIONS OF SPEECH STIMULI ON THE RECOGNITION AND RETENTION OF WORDS BY APHASIC ADULTS BY William R. DOpheide A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State university in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences 1968 Gfi/‘Vé/ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer is indebted to a number of people whose guidance, assistance, and cooperation made the completion of this study possible. Their interest in this study is gratefully acknowledged. To Drs. Herbert J. Oyer, Leo V. Deal, and Bradley Lashbrook, staff members of the Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences at Michigan State University, for their guidance. To Miss Josephine Simonson (University of Michigan), Mr. William Locke (American Legion Hospital, Battle Creek), Mr. Pat Sacco (Constance Brown Hearing and Speech Center), Mr. Charles Hillary (VA HOSpital, Battle Creek), and Dr. Clyde Willis (Western Michigan University) for their assistance in obtaining subjects. To the aphasic adults for their willing participa- tion in the tasks of this investigation. To my family, Marjorie, Kathleen, Marcia, and Mark for bearing an indescribable responsibility for the com- pletion of this study. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FI LIST OF AP Chapter I. INT GURES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . PENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . RODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Purpose of the Study . . . . . . . . . Importance of the Study. . . . . . . . Definitions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Organization of the Report . . . . . . II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE. . . . . . . . . . . III. EXP IV. RES Development of Concepts of Aphasia . . Neurophysiological Rationale . . . . . Clinical Basis of Investigation. . . . Experimental Tasks and Associated Variables. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ERIMENTAL PROCEDURES . . . . . . . . . Selection and Preparation of Stimulus Material . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Equipment and Materials. . . . . . . . Recording of Auditory Stimuli. . . . . Selection and Grouping of Subjects . . Presentation of Stimuli to Subjects. . ULTS AND DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . Results from Word Recognition Task . . Results from Retention Span Task . . . Results from Inter-task Comparison . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAP APPENDICES Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . SuggeStions for Future Research. . . . HY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 iii Page ii iv vi Nahum t—' 14 24 33 37 48 51 51 58 59 67 76 84 89 102 105 107 113 113 116 118 121 127 LIST OF TABLES 1% 1. Separation of PPVT Forms into Four Lists and Three Hearing Vocabulary Levels . . . . . . . . . 2. Comparison of Auditory Language Disturbance (ALD) Error Scores of Two Subject Groups. . . . . 3. Comparison of Age, Educational Level, and Elapsed Time since Onset of Aphasia of Two Subject Groups 4. Sample Arrangement of Lists with Block Index Numbers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5. Summary of Analysis of Variance Comparing the Effects of Hearing Vocabulary Levels, Subject Groups, and Stimulus Duration on the Word Recognition Task. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6. Hearing Vocabulary Level Means, and Differences between Means for Each Auditory Language Dis- turbance Group. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7. Mean Performance for Each Auditory Language Disturbance Group at Four Stimulus Durations, and Within Group Mean Differences between Duration Conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8. Summary of Analysis of Variance Comparing the Effects of Subject Groups and Stimulus Durations on the Retention Span Task. . . . . . . . . . . . 9. Spearman Rank Correlations of Performance on the Word Recognition Task and Retention Span Task for the Two Subject Groups . . . . . . . . . iv Page 52 72 74 80 91 93 100 104 106 Figure 1. Graph of Group Means and Total Group Mean at Three HVL . . . . . . . . 2. Graph of Means for Hearing Vocabulary Levels of Groups at Four Durations. 3. Schematic of Speech Expansion Device . 4. Expansion Process: LIST OF FIGURES vs. Percentage of Expansion. Tape Transport Velocity Page 97 99 128 132 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix Page A. Further Description of Process by which Stimulus Durations Were Increased . . . . . . 128 B. Span of Retention Response Plates and Associated Stimuli. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 C. Data Used to Establish Acceptance Intervals for Durations of Words in Original Recording. 139 D. Calibration Check of Expanded Tapes . . . . . 144 E. Scores of Subjects on Recognition and Retention Tasks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 vi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Man can experience no more abrupt loSS of his uniqueness than to lose his ability to communicate. For as Gibran has written When you meet your friend, . . . let the Spirit in you move your lips and direct your tongue. Let the voice within your voice speak to the ear of his ear; For his soul will keep the truth of your heart as the taste of the wine is remembered When the colour is forgotten and the vessel is no more.1 To experience disability of the "voice within your voice" is to suffer deprivation in perhaps the most essential and sustaining of human behaviors. As Goldstein has stated The different views regarding the nature of man reflect themselves in the discussions of man's out- standing capacity: language.2 The abrupt disruption of this capacity is aphasia. It is a language disorder associated with cerebral insult and as such is very frequently accompanied by other phy- sical and behavioral disabilities. 1Kahil Gibran, The Prophet (1923) (New York: Alfred A. Knopf, Inc., 1964), p. 61. 2Kurt Goldstein, Language and Language Disturbances (New York: Grune and Stratton, 1948), p. 345. Aphasia has been recorded in the literature over the centuries.1 However, as man's nature has lead him to increase his life expectancy, to improve his means of deal- ing with disease and disability, and to invent more traumatic ways of doing harm, the problems of the aphasic adult have drawn increased attention. Within the last 100 years, over 30,000 books and articles have dealt with the problems of the aphasic adult.2 In this time, Esti Freud has seen the investigations as taking three distinct approaches: direct anatomical-physiological investigations; psychological observations of the total behavior of the patient from which central nervous system dysfunction might be inferred; and structural, linguistic analyses of the language behavior itself.3 The responsibilities of Speech pathology in this interdisciplinary approach have been centered, of course, on the language behavior itself. The increased demands for language rehabilitation services during and after World War II4 seem to have intensified the search for better 1Arthur L. Benton and Robert J. Joynt, "Early Descriptions of Aphasia," AMA Archives of Neurology, III (1960), pp. 205-22. 2Esti D. Freud, "Recent Trends in Aphasia," American Journal of Psychiatry, CX (1953), p. 186. 31bid.. pp. 186-93. 4Joseph M. Wepman, Recovery from Aphasia (New York: Ronald Press Company, 1951), p. 4. diagnostic and treatment procedures with which to assist the aphasic adult. It was from this clinical viewpoint that the present experimental investigation was undertaken. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was to examine the effec- tiveness of a treatment procedure used by clinicians in language therapy with aphasic adults. The study was directed at investigating, experimentally, the influence of increased durations of selected Speech stimuli on the receptive lan- guage performance of aphasic adults. “Six: questions con- cerning aphasic adults were posed for investigation. 1. What influence does increasing the duration of Speech stimuli have on the performance of an auditory word recognition task? 2. Is there a relationship between the meaning- fulness of the Speech stimuli and the influence of increases in stimulus duration on performance of an auditory word recognition task? 3. Does the influence of increased durations of the speech stimuli vary as a function of the severity of the subject's disturbance in auditory language reception on tasks requiring the auditory recognition of words? 4. What influence does increasing the duration of the speech stimuli have on the performance of a retention span task in which words are presented auditorily and in series? 5. Does the influence of increased durations of the Speech stimuli vary in relation to the severity of the subject's disturbance in audi- tory language reception on a task requiring the retention of stimuli presented auditorily? 6. What relationship exists between performance on an auditory word recognition task and an auditory retention span task at several levels of increased stimulus duration? Importance of the Study It has been said ". . . in the investigation of problems as complex as aphasia almost any question one could raise would be worth study."1 This subcommittee of the American Speech and Hearing Association, studying the research needs in aphasia, was indicating the strong need for investigating any and all aspects of the clinical management of the language problems of the aphasic adult. This subcommittee did indicate the Significance of questions related to the differential effectiveness of therapeutic lWalter Amster, g£_§l,, "V. Report of Subcommittee on Problems of Aphasia," in Research Needs in Speech Pathol- ogy and Audiology (Washington: American Speech and Hearing Association, 1959), p. 42. processes with different aphasic categories.l These different categories are not the classical categories of the last century which are being perpetuated today by psychological and neurological textbooks.2 Major investigations within the last thirty years have denied strongly the classical dichotomy between sensory and motor language deficits. With increasing emphasis on the objective assessment of the language behavior itself, concern has focused on the general nature of the aphasic language deficit and the definition of its dimensions which have diagnostic and treatment significance.3 Two facts have made the definition of aphasia dif- ficult. The disabilities of the aphasic adult, stemming as they do from some cerebral insult, require the profes- sional attention of several disciplines.4 Secondly, attempt- ing to understand disordered language function, masked by other disabling physical and psychological problems, is extremely difficult, eSpecially when one considers that normal language processes are only now becoming clearer lIbid. 2James J. Jenkins and Hildred Schuell, "Changing Concepts of Aphasia," Perceptual and Motor Skills, XIII (1961). p. 270. 3Charles E. Osgood and Murray S. Miron, Approaches to the Study of Aphasia (Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1963), p. 126. 41bid., p. 3. through scientific investigation.1 The definition of aphasia adopted for use in this study was one derived from extended empirical research by Schuell and Jenkins.2 This definition, which recognizes a general dimension of language, has re- ceived a good deal of clinical interest since it operation-I alizes the concept of aphasia in terms which are quite amenable to clinical management in language therapy. The two major characteristics of aphasia given in this defini- tion suggested clear-cut tasks upon which the experimental variable could be tested. Recognizing the coexistence of some degree of both receptive and expressive disability in all patients, there appears to be rather substantial support for the Opinion that auditory language problems are generally an aspect of the total language dysfunction of the aphasic adult. As Schuell has stated Probably all aphasic patients Show some impairment of auditory processes, because language, learned by ear, remains dependent upon discrimination, recognition, and recall of learned auditory patterns, and upon audi- tory feedback processes. lIbid. 2Hildred Schuell and James J. Jenkins, "The Nature of the Language Deficit in Aphasia," Psychological Review, LXVI (1959), pp. 50-51. James J. Jenkins and Hildred Schuell, "Further Work on Language Deficit in Aphasia,“ Psychological Review, LXXI (1964), pp. 87—93. 3Hildred Schuell, Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia with the Minnesota Test (Minneapolis: university of Minnesota Press, 1965), p. 5. It appeared that any investigation of techniques for facili— tating auditory language reception would have relevance to the broad range of problems in aphasia. Explaining improved language function in aphasia on the basis of treatment procedures is difficult Since there is insufficient knowledge of cerebral function in such complex functions as language.1 However, there have been several theories of cerebral function advanced which give reason to believe certain modifications of an auditory language stimulus may elicit improved recognition or reten— tion of the linguistic significance of the stimulus,2 while other current theories would suggest no improvement from such modifications of the stimulus itself.3 The particular modification of the auditory lan- guage stimuli to be studied is increased duration. Clinical practice has indicated that giving greater temporal value to the phonemes of a word is a means of improving its lOsgood and Miron, Approaches to Aphasia, pp. 33-37. 2Aleksandor Romanovich Luria, Higher Cortical Func- tions in Man, trans. by Basil Haigh (New YOrk: Basic Books, Inc., 1966), pp. 97-103. Wilder Penfield and Lamar Roberts, Speech and Brain Mechanisms (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1959), pp. 21-22. Karl Pribram, "Neo- cortical Function in Behavior," in Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior, eds. Harry F. Harlow and Clinton N. Woolsey (Madison: university of Wisconsin Press, 1958), pp. 167-168. 3Norman Geschwind, "Disconnexion Syndromes in Ani- mals and Man, Parts I and II," Brain, LXXXVIII (1965), pp. 237-293 and 585-644. recognition by aphasic adults.l Other variables which have appeared to influence the adequacy of the stimulus for the patient were recognized and were considered in evaluating the influence of the experimental variable. The availabil- ity of a device capable of producing these expansions in word duration2 offered promise of providing the speech pathologist with another clinical tool. It seemed worth- while to make a test of its potential effectiveness in the language rehabilitation of the aphasic adult. Definitions Aphasia.--This nonfunctional language disturbance is characterized by a reduction or loss of a centralized ability to cope with the arbitrary meanings, associations, and relationships which exist in an individual's system of communication. This basically unidimensional behavior may be further impaired by independent but related trans- missive dysfunctions, variously referred to as agnosias, apraxias, or sensorimotor deficits. lHildred Schuell, James J. Jenkins and Edward Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults: Diagnosis, Prognosis, and Treatment (New York: Harper and Row, Publishers, 1964), pp. 340-41. 2Hugh 8. Allen, Jr., "The Eltro Information Rate Changer - Mark II: Simple Quality Speech Compression" (paper presented at Conference on Compressed Speech, Uhi- versity of Louisville, October 19, 1966). While this definition was drawn from several the- oretical positions regarding the nature of the disorder, it is admittedly biased in the direction of the Schuell and Jenkins definition which characterizes aphasia as a reduction of available vocabulary, impaired verbal retention span, and impaired perception and production of messages. The impaired perception and production of messages, they have stated, may be secondary to the impairment of avail- able vocabulary and verbal retention Span. The definition recognized also, the contributions of Wepman, §£_§l,, who have viewed aphasia as a disruption in the central nervous system's capacity to integrate lan— guage stimuli once such stimuli are free of their input modalities, with the disruption having the potential of falling at any point in the process; in the arousal of mean- ing, in the semantic word selection, or in the syntactic process. Subjects in this study were identified as "aphasic" on the basis of competent clinical diagnosis employing one or both of the following recognized diagnostic tools: the Language Modalities Test for Aphasia3 and The Minnesota lSchuell, Jenkins and Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 113. 2Joseph M. Wepman, §£_§l,, "Studies in Aphasia: Background and Theoretical Formulations," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders (JSHD), XXV (1960), p. 328. 3Joseph M. Wepman and Lyle V. Jones, The Langugge Modalities Test for Aphasia (Chicago: Education-Industry Service, 1961). -10- Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia.l Hence, "aphasia" was Operationalized on the basis of performance on one or both of these tests. Auditory language disturbance.—-Deficits in ability to deal with receptive language, auditorily, was defined on the basis of a subject's score on seven subtests of the Auditory Disturbances Section of the Minnesota Test.2 The subtests used from this section of the test were as follows: recognizing common words; discriminating between paired words; recognizing letters; identifying items named serially; following directions; repeating digits; and repeating sen- tences. All of the stimulus material for these tasks were presented auditorily. It was reported, on the basis of a factor analysis, that all these tests loaded heavily on a general language factor, indicating the importance of auditory processes in 3 language. Meaningfulness: levels of hearingyvocabulary. The meaningfulness of a word was determined by the develop- mental period in which the word was expected to have become lHildred Schuell, Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia: Administrative Manual (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 1965). 21bid.. pp. 5-8. 3Hildred Schuell, Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia, p. 25. -11.. a part of subject's hearing vocabulary, i.e., his auditory receptive vocabulary. Those words from the earliest develop- mental period were considered more meaningful than words from the later developmental period. The word stimuli used were those appearing in the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test.1 This test provided an empirically derived set of words, scaled in terms of the chronological develOpment of hearing vocabulary through at least the first eighteen years of life.2 On the basis of age group data, the stimulus words of the test were arranged in the empirically—determined order of difficulty. The order of difficulty was based solely on the number of correct responses rendered by successively older age groups. A subject's hearing vocabulary was assessed on the basis of his word recognition score obtained on the Peabody Test; it was assumed the subject recognized the stimulus word if he correctly identified the response picture asso- ciated with the stimulus word. Levels of hearing vocabulary were defined by arbitrarily dividing the scaled set of words into three groups. The division points were at words indexing mean expectations for five year old subjects, and for subjects approximately 15 years-6 months of age. This lLloyd Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Expanded Manual (Minneapolis: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1965). 2 Ibid., p. 25. -12- division assigned test items 1—50 to Level I, items 51-100 to Level II, and items 101-150 to Level III. By determining the number of correct responses made by the subject within each of the three levels, his available hearing vocabulary for single words was analyzed in terms of words associated with the initial, intermediate, and late phases of the early development of hearing vocabulary development. Increased stimulus duration.--As used in this study, increased stimulus duration referred to increasing the temporal value of phonemic segments of the words. By a process to be described, the increases were produced as a percentage of the duration of the word as originally recorded. Three nominal levels of increase were employed: 125%, 150%, and 175% increase. Hence, if a word (as originally recorded) had a duration of 500 msec., it would approximate 750 msec. at the 150% level of increased duration. This processing is frequently referred to as expanded Speech. Organization of the Report In Chapter I, the direction in which concepts of aphasia have moved in recent years has been indicated, and the Special concerns and interests of Speech patholo- gists have been identified. The questions posed by this study were presented and related to the needs of the aphasic adult in his efforts to regain better language -13- function. In addition, terms of particular importance to this study are defined. Chapter II consists of a comprehensive review of literature pertinent to: (1) the development of current concepts of aphasia, (2) the neurophysiological basis for the possible outcomes from the experimental treatment, (3) clinical observations which suggested the selection of the experimental variable, and (4) matters related to the selection, design and development of the language tasks and the preparation of the stimulus materials. The following tOpics are discussed in Chapter III: procedures employed in develOping materials for the experi- mental tasks: selection and grouping of subjects; and pro- cedures followed in presenting the tasks to the subjects. Included also is a discussion of procedures used in verify- ing the expanded durations of the stimulus material, plus a description of the characteristics of the two groups of subjects. In Chapter IV the results of the statistical analyses are discussed in terms of the null hypotheses generated from the six questions stated in Chapter I. The results are discussed in terms of relevant theoretical notions, clinical observations and research findings. Chapter V summarizes the present study and presents conclusions that can be drawn within the limitations imposed. Recommendations for future research are suggested. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE This chapter presents a review of the theoretical, clinical and research literature relevant to this inves- tigation. Beginning with a consideration of the develop- ment of understandings of aphasia, the importance of auditory language reception problems in aphasia is indicated. The discussion focuses on theories of neurophysiological func- tion that might account for the possible outcomes of the experimental treatment employed in this investigation. This review reports the clinical observations that prompted the investigation, as well as factors reported to influence auditory language reception in adult aphasics. The chapter concludes with a consideration of reports concerning the process employed to produce increases in the duration of the stimuli. DevelOpment of Concepts of Aphasia The history of the study of aphasia has been marked by a great deal of variation in the concepts concerning the nature of the language disorder. This is not at all unusual when one considers the comment of Osgood and Miron, who -14- -15- observe that . . . the problems of aphasia represented a natural intersection of the interests of neurologists, clin- ical psychologists, linguists, Speech pathologists, and experimental psychologists specializing in language. Even a simplified definition of aphasia as "disturbance of language behavior" gives balanced weight to these fields: "disturbance" (neurology and clinical psychology), "language" (linguistics and speech pathology), and "behavior" (experimental psychology).1 While the early reports appear to have been largely descrip— tive medical accounts, later efforts tried to explain the language deficit in terms of an underlying neurological dysfunction. In more recent years, emphasis appears to have Shifted to a more systematic investigation, aimed at describing the actual language deficits in ways meaning- ful to the treatment of the language disorder itself. In the brief review that follows, the intention is to indicate the direction in which the concepts of aphasia have moved in a little over 100 years. The review reflects the development of the realization that any concept of the language disturbances in aphasia must include considera- tion of both receptive and expressive language functions. Early develOpments.—-A ccnsiderable amount of de- scriptive clinical material had been accumulated up to the beginning of the nineteenth century. This was the conclusion 1Charles E. Osgood and Murray S. Miron, Approaches to the Study of Aphasia (Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1963), p. 3. ~16- reached by Benton and Joyntl after their reasonably extensive review of literature dating from Hippocratic writing to 1800. They felt that the writers of this period (largely physicians) had recorded a rather broad range of aphasic disturbances, with one notable exception. Other than one reference, Benton and Joynt found no mention of specific impairment in speech comprehension as an aSpect of aphasic disturbances. Benton and Joynt state . . . sensory aphasia remained virtually unrecognized as a Specific language deficit for another three- quarters of a century, until the appearance of Wernicke's monograph in 1874.2 Wernicke regarded the disturbance discussed by Broca3 as a motor aphasia. Wernicke proposed another type of language disturbance which he labelled sensory aphasia. He reasoned that, while the two language deficits (sensory and motor aphasia) could exist as separate aSpects of an aphasic disturbance, he was not convinced that in pure motor aphasia the ability to understand speech always remained unimpaired. The coexistence of the two deficits he seemed to ascribe to another concept of conduction aphasia, about which he had some uncertainty. Wernicke defined sensory 1Arthur L. Benton and Robert J. Joynt, "Early Descriptions of Aphasia," AMA Archives of Neurology, III (1960). pp. 205-22. 21bid., p. 220. 3Jules Kann, "A Translation of Broca's Original Article on the Location of the Speech Center," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders (JSHD), XV (1950), pp. 16-20. -17- aphasia as loss of understanding of Speech, with hearing and articulation intact, and writing severely disturbed. He stated that sensory aphasia resulted from temporal lobe lesions in the area of the auditory center of the first temporal convolution--"Wernicke's area" in classical neur- ology.l A little over a decade later in 1887, Kussmaul had formulated a definition of aphasia which seemed to accommodate more comfortably the sensory and motor aspects of language. To understand aphasia, he wrote, it was important to understand the development of Speech in its linguistic and utilitarian contexts. He defined aphasia as "the impairment of execution, expression or understanding of any symbols by which one man communicates his ideas or feelings to another."2 With this broad view of the problem, it is not surprising to find that Kussmaul did not support a strict neurological localization point of view, as did Broca and Wernicke. Hughlings Jackson, initiating some thirty years lCarl Wernicke, "Symptom-Complex of Aphasia (German- 1874)," in Diseases of the Nervous System, ed. by A. Church (New York: Appleton, 1908), pp. 265-324, cited by Hildred Schuell, James J. Jenkins and Edward Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults: Diagnosisy Prognosisy and Treatment (New York: Harper & Row, Publishers, 1964), p. 17. 2Adolph Kussmaul, "Disturbances of Speech: An Attempt in the Pathology of Speech," in Cyclopedia of the Practice of Medicine, ed. by H. vonZiemssen (New York: Frank Wood, 1887), pp. 581—870. -18- of careful clinical observation at about this same time, was also unable to accept popular theories of localization. While his work was not widely recognized until the early twentieth century, he made some very significant contribu- tions to the study of this disorder.1 Jackson's study of aphasia caused him to search for a dynamic concept of cerebral function which involved the dependency of lower functional centers upon higher levels of functional organ- ization. He was aware of the distinct receptive language deficit manifested by aphasic patients, although he con- centrated more attention on deficits in expressive language. However, Jackson certainly perceived the reciprocal rela- tionship between the receptive and the expressive aspects in aphasic disorders. He discussed the apparent word loss of the aphasic as more a matter of words not being avail- able for prOpositional expression than an absolute loss of the words.2 Recent developmentS.--It appeared that the first recognition (in the United States) that aphasia was neither purely receptive nor purely expressive was in the 1935 lTheodore Weisenberg and Katherine E. McBride, Aphasia: A Clinical and Psyghological Study (originally published 1935), (New York: Hafner Publishing Company, Inc., 1964), p. 11. 2J. Hughlings Jackson, The Selected Writings (2 vols.; New YOrk: Basic Books, Inc., 1958), II, pp. 365—392. -19- work of Weisenberg and McBride.l In introducing their fourfold division of aphasic disorders, they state . . . it must be emphasized that none of the terms is other than descriptive and that none does more than point out the most marked characteristics of the case. Neither patients of the eXpressive nor those of the receptive group are handicapped solely in expressive functions or solely in receptive. The disorders are predominantly expressive or receptive; the other pro- cesses in either case are always more or less affected. 2 The four classifications of expressive, receptive, expressive- receptive, and amnesic were derived from an empirical study of sixty aphasic patients. While there was a good deal to criticize in this study,3 the explicit statement concerning the coexistence of receptive and expressive deficits is noteworthy. It seemed Weisenberg and McBride were more explicit in this regard than was Head.4 Head also avoided the sensory-motor dichotomy in his classifications of verbal, syntactical, and nominal. It will be noted that Head's observations lead him to a linguistically oriented classification. Closer to the present time, one finds a nomencla- ture for aphasic language disturbances in Eisenson's work lWeisenberg and McBride, Aphasia, pp. 145-149. 2Ibid., p. 144. 3Hildred Schuell, James J. Jenkins and Edward Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults: Diagnosisy Prognosisy and Treatment (New York: Harper and Row, Publishers, 1964). pp. 41-42. 4Henry Head, Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech (2 vols.; New York: Macmillan, 1926), I, pp. 221-68. -20- which is apparently a modification of the Weisenberg and McBride work. Eisensonl avoided the sensory-motor dichot- omy in proposing the following four classes: predominantly evaluative (receptive or sensory); predominantly productive (expressive); subsymbolic disturbances on evaluative side (agnosias); and subsymbolic disturbances on the productive side (apraxias and dysarthrias). Pointing out that these categories were the predominant area of disturbance, not pure disturbances, he suggested that this classification system points to the area in which the major treatment emphasis Should be focused. It is worth noting that Eisenson allowed, in his nomenclature, for separate behavioral defi- cits of agnosia, apraxia, and dysarthria. This recognition of nonlanguage disabilities, coexisting with aphasia, has been reflected in other recent theoretical positions. Present status.--In 1960, Wepman, 3p 31.,2 develOped their formulation of aphasia as a disruption in the central nervous system's capacity to integrate language stimuli once such stimuli are free of their input modalities, with the disruption having the potential of falling at any point lJon Eisenson, Examining for Aphasia (rev. ed.; New York: Psychological Corporation, 1954), pp. 12-19. 2Joseph M. Wepman, §p_gl,, "Studies in Aphasia: Background and Theoretical Formulations," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXV (1960), pp. 323-32. -21- in the process. They have suggested such points as being in the arousal of meaning, in the semantic word selection process, or in the syntactic process. They regarded agnosias and apraxias as nonsymbolic transmissive disorders related to receptive and expressive language modalities, while the aphasias were viewed as disruptions in the symbolic language process. In setting out to research this concept of aphasia, Wepman and Jones develOped the Language Modalities Test for Aphasia (LMTA)1. This test was designed to explore not only the stimulus-response relationship along common input and output modalities but also three types of linguistic involvement commonly found in aphasic patients. Their research indicated, to them, that one way to differentiate among symbolic disorders associated with central nervous system impairment was to classify them in terms of the loss or dysfunction in one or more of three processes. The three processes that they defined and labeled were pragmatic aphasia, semantic aphasia and syntatic aphasia. They added a fourth category (jargon aphasia) for patients generally unintelligible; and a fifth category (global aphasia) for patients unable to produce a real verbal response to stimuli. From a study by Spiegel, Jones and 1Joseph M. Wepman and Lyle V. Jones, The Language Modalities Test for Aphasia (Chicago: Education-Industry Service, 1961). -22- Wepman,l it was concluded that aphasic patients evaluated with the LMTA were distributed along a continuum of general language disability. In addition, the patients also dis- tributed themselves along other dimensions indicative of Specific losses in more specialized language abilities related to input and output modalities. The theoretical position that most strongly supports the concept of a general language deficit in aphasia is expressed by Schuell, Jenkins, and Jimenez-Pabon.2 They have viewed aphasia as a general language deficit crossing all language modalities and as a deficit that may or may not be further complicated by impairment of auditory, visual and sensorimotor processes. In fifteen years of empirical, systematic observations of some 1000 aphasic patients, these investigators saw the language deficit itself as characterized by a reduction of available vocabulary, im- paired verbal retention Span, and impaired perception and production of messages. The impaired perception and pro- duction of messages, they have stated, may be secondary to the impairment of available vocabulary and verbal retention Span. lDouglas K. Spiegel, Lyle V. Jones, Joseph M. Wepman, "Test Responses as Predictors of Free-Speech Characteristics in Aphasic Patients," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research (JSHR). VIII (1965), pp. 349-362. Schuell, Jenkins, and Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults. PP. 113-133. -23- A major outcome of the work of Schuell and her associates was the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia,l a test intended to provide an operational description of aphasia through observation of the level at which language performance breaks down in each of the prin- cipal language modalities. This, Schuell stated,2 is essen- tially what there is to observe in aphasia. A section of the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia was used in this study. This review of several major contributions supported the position that an investigation of an aspect of the receptive auditory language ability of adult aphasics was an investigation having relevance to general problems of aphasia. Receptive language and expressive language cannot be dichotomized, and the auditory modality is certainly a principal receptive language modality. Among the more recent descriptive studies of aphasia, the empirically derived concepts presented by Schuell and her associates provided the basis for this investigation of the influence of increased durations of Speech stimuli on the receptive language performance of aphasics. Their lHildred Schuell, Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia with the Minnesota Test (Minneapolis: University of Minn- esota Press, 1965). 2Schuell, Jenkins, and Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 170. -24- two principal characteristics of the language deficit in aphasia, a reduction in available vocabulary and an impair- ment in verbal retention Span, suggested the two tasks on which the experimental variable will be tested. Neurophysiological Rationale While this study was primarily concerned with the language behavior of aphasics, an attempt was made to find some neurOphysiological basis for the possible influences of the increased durations of the auditory stimuli. Several current theories of the functional organization of the cere- brum were explored. One of these seemed to predict no positive effect, and three which appeared to offer some suggestion of a positive effect from increasing the dura- tion of the stimuli. At the outset it was realized the task could not be completed with a high degree of clarity, since the perceptual and cognitive aSpectS of auditory language reception were still cloaked in too much uncertainty, ambiguity and conflicting Opinion. Yet the undertaking seemed a necessary preliminary step in this investigation. Localization position.--A present day reflection of more classical neurophysiological concepts appeared in the work of Geschwind.l Geschwind has presented a rather lNorman Geschwind, "Disconnexion Syndromes in Ani- mals and Man, Parts I and II," Brain, LXXXVIII (1965), pp. 277-291, and pp. 604-44. -25- extensive discussion of disconnection syndromes and their influence on language behavior. In addition to dealing with disruptions in connections between primary and secondary (or associative) areas of the cortex, he placed importance on the inferior parietal lobule (roughly Brodmann's areas 39 and 40) as a key site in cortical integration. This area is uniquely develOped in man, Geschwind points out. Not essentially concentric with any of the primary sensory or motor projection areas, this area--"the association area of association areas"--may be capable of powerful intermodal associations. The loss or reduction of intermodal associa- tions between audition, vision and some kinesthetic sensa- tions would undoubtedly lead to impaired language function. He discussed the disconnections in terms of both intra- hemispheric and inter-hemiSpheric fibers. From Geschwind's position, it appeared that once the connections are dis- rupted the resulting loss or attenuation of function is permanent and irreversible. The mediating influence of the "association area of association areas" over the transactions of the sensory and motor areas is no longer as effective or efficient as it was prior to the cerebral insult. It seemed that, from the vieWpoint of Geschwind's transcortical model, the experimental treatment of the speech stimuli in this study would have no positive influ- ence on the aphasic subject's ability to recognize or retain the stimuli. -26- Moderate localizationyposition.--An orientation toward cerebral function that appeared to hold more promise in terms of the experimental treatment is the Pavlovian reflex theory of sensation and his concept of cortical "analyzers." These views have been investigated and set forth by Luria, a very thorough presentation appearing in 1962.1 According to Luria, the reflex theory of sensation . . . is always an active reflex process associated with the selection of the essential (Signal) compon- ents of the stimuli and the inhibition of the nonessen- tial, subsidiary components. It always incorporates effector mechanisms leading to the tuning of the peri- pheral receptor apparatus and reSponSible for carrying out the selective reactions to determine the signal components of the stimulus. It envisages a continuous process of increased excitability in respect to some components of the stimulus and of decreased excitabil- ity in respect to others . . . In other words sensation incorporates the_process of analysis and synthesis of signals while they are still in the first stages of arrival . . . The units of any sensory process (includ- ing hearing) are not only acts of reception of individual signals, measurable in terms of thresholds of sensation, but also acts of complex analysis and integration of signals, measurable in units of comparison and discrim- ination. The cortical region having to do with the most precise differentiation and the most complex integration of audi- tory stimuli was part of the "nuclear zone“ for audition located in the superior temporal subregion. The "nuclear lAleksandor Romanovick Luria, Higher Cortical Func- tions in Man, trans. by Basil Haigh, (New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1966). 2Ibid., p. 97. -27- zone" was defined as Brodmann's Areas 41, 42 and 22.1 Injuries to Area 41, the primary area of the analyzer, have produced marked decreases in detection and discrimination of auditory stimuli. However, lesions in the secondary areas (Brodmann's 42 and 22) have resulted in quite differ- ent auditory dysfunction, according to Luria. These lesions, in the left hemisphere, have lead to disorganization in the ability to cope with complex groups of stimuli and the relationships among component parts of complex stimuli. This dysfunction in the "analytic-synthetic" capa- city is said to lead to difficulty in the identification of the phonemic signs in the acoustic patterns of the language system.2 Because the "auditory analyzer" is closely asso- ciated with the cortical apparatuses of kinesthetic (arti- culatory) analysis, the efferent link for the perception of the sounds of Speech was not sufficiently activated. These secondary areas possess wider systemic connections with adjacent cortical areas than does the primary auditory area. Direct stimulation of the secondary areas has given rise to potentials detected in the inferior segments of the premotor frontal portions of the cortex--Brodmann's Areas 44, 46 and 10. There was reason to suspect that the experimental lIbid., p. 44. 2Ibid., pp. 102-03. -28- treatment might activate sufficiently the post-cerebral insult capability of these secondary areas. Luria has cited studies that indicate the induction of potentials in these secondary auditory areas requires more intensive stimula- tion of the peripheral receptor than is required to induce potentials in the primary auditory area.1 It seemed the increased durations of the Speech stimuli might provide just such an intensification of the phonemic elements. Through the neurophysiological principles of facilitation and sum- mation, this intensification may have improved perception and enabled better recognition and retention of the word stimuli. It was important to recognize that just a moderate increase in the sensory aspect of perception might be suffi- cient to trigger the efferent (motor) aspect of the percep- tual action, hence sharpening the subject's perception of the stimulus to the point where linguistic meaning was aroused. Subcortical integration.--A second basis for hypo- thesizing a positive influence from the increased durations of the stimuli on the tasks of this investigation seemed to be found in theories of cerebral function which deempha- sized transcortical pathways. Reducing reliance on the extent of post-injury intactness of cortical pathways seemed lIbid., p. 99. -29- to place less demand on the residual, functional adequacy of both the primary cortical areas and the associative cor- tical areas. It appeared that the intensification or am- plification of the phonemic elements would have a greater likelihood of positively influencing performance if the residual adequacy of both primary and associative areas could be maximally integrated without depending heavily on the extent of the intactness of transcortical pathways. Two theories of functional integration of the cere- bral cortex seemed to offer such a basis. They were the theories advanced by Penfield and Roberts and by Pribram. Penfield and Roberts1 emphasized cortical-subcortical relationships in the matter of cortical integration. They relegated transcortical connections to a secondary role. They have drawn, as support for this position, evidence derived from the functional adequacy of patients who have experienced radical excision of cortical tissue bordering on primary sensory and primary motor areas. They have named this system the "centrencephalic system" and defined it roughly as a ganglionic area of the higher brain which acts as the central integrating mechanism--the point at which integration of neuronal transactions reaches its peak.2 lWilder Penfield and Lamar Roberts, Speech and Brain Mechanisms (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton university Press, 1959). 2Ibid.. pp. 21-22. -30- This postulated interdependency of subcortical centers and cortical centers, they suggested, is a complex one. Impulses triggered in subcortical areas take on the aspects of cortically bound efferent impulses. As efferent impulses aimed at the cortex, they activate one or more cortical areas. The activated cortical areas provide a mediating influence over the impulses having subcortical origin. In this mediating function, certain modifications may be made before the patterned (or repatterned) impulse complex travels along the post-cortical leg of the pathways to the peripheral effector mechanism. In the view of Penfield and Roberts, subcortical integration functionally unites the two cortical areas they have indicated enable the elaboration of language through speech. The most important of the two areas for language function is the Posterior Speech Cortex, which encompasses Brodmann's Areas 39 and 40. This is the area to which Geschwind attached great significance in terms of intermodal associations. The Posterior Speech Cortex also encompasses most of the classical Wernicke's area. Of secondary importance to language function was the Anterior Speech Cortex Situated in the frontal lobe, encompassing Broca's area, Area 44.1 It will be recalled that Luria reports radiations from secondary acoustic fields reach lIbid.. pp. 192-204. -31- Area 44. While these points suggested areas of correspon- dence in the three rationales, a very important and dis- tinguishing difference rested in the reliance Penfield and Roberts placed on subcortical integration. Pribraml has proposed an alternative to transcor- tical models of cerebral organization in cognitive processes. He cited evidence from several sources which, he believed, made transcortical models unsatisfactory. One type of evidence indicated the classical dichotomy between motor (efferent) and sensory (afferent) cortical areas was not valid.2 Secondly, while the first issue might be solved by designating the areas "primary motor (or sensory) pro- jection areas," the functional relationship of these primary projection areas to their association areas was not sub- stantiated by various surgical intervention techniques applied to the cortex. Again, the transcortical model becomes less useful when it was observed that the behavioral changes following such removal, isolation or cross-hatching of the cortex were not those that would be predicted by the transcortical models.3 Pribram suggested an alternative model based on 1Karl H. Pribram, "Neocortical Function in Behavior," in Biological and Biochemical Phases of Behavior (Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1958), pp. 151-72. 2Ibid., pp. 155—158. 3Ibid.. pp. 159-166. -32- the thalamocortical connection that the cortical area has with the thalamus. If the thalamic nucleus from which the thalamocortical connection arose received inputs from outside (extrinsic) the thalamus, the cortical area was regarded as an extrinsic sector. Intrinsic sectors of the cortex are those sectors having thalamocortical connections originat- ing in thalamic nuclei which received inputs from within (intrinsic) the thalamus. Pribram reported the extrinsic sectors correSpond to the motor-sensory projection areas, whereas the intrinsic sectors were the classical association areas. With direct functional connections between projection areas and asso- ciations being doubtful, the functional Significance of the subdivisions of the intrinsic cortex was believed to be the convergence of their output on a common subcortical mechanism. This mechanism also receives output from the extrinsic sectors. The output from the intrinsic system was seen as influencing the output of the extrinsic sectors through this convergence.l Without denying the specificity of function associated with certain primary and association areas, a model of cortical function resembling Penfield and Roberts' "centrencephalic system was developed." In both the Penfield-Roberts and Pribram concep- tions, a hypothesized system was seen which seems more lIbid.. pp. 167-168. -33- resilient and capable of compensating for injury. Working from these systems, there appeared to be some reason to suppose that Speech stimuli, in which phonemic durations were increased, would have some positive consequences on the auditory functions of aphasic subjects. Luria's theor- etical views might encompass such an outcome, but a posi- tive influence from the intended stimuli would be somewhat less probable. From Geschwind's point of view, the pre- diction of no significant effect from the expanded speech stimuli appeared to be the most logical. Clinical Basis of Investigation The short history of language therapy for the aphasic dates back to the period immediately after World War I and seemed to have made its most significant advance since World War II in response to the needs of those who sustained brain injury in that war.1 From his experiences in treating aphasics during World War II, Wepman reported the major receptive disability of the aphasic adult was in the comprehension of the Spoken word. The most common deficit in this modality, which he labeled "auditory verbal agnosia," resembled the sensory aphasia described by Wernicke. However, Wepman reported lJoseph Wepman, Recovery from Aphasia (New York: Ronald Press Company, 1951), p. 4. -34- . . . patients showing this problem have all grada- tions between comparatively normal Speech production and a total lack of that facility, between a total lack of comprehension and a partial lack of the ability to understand spoken words, and between a retained ability to imitate and an apparent total lack of imi- tative ability.l After necessarily ruling out the influence of impaired hearing acuity and general intellectual impairment, the comprehension loss remains, indicating the cortical disabil- ity of integrating aural stimuli, he stated. Facilitating auditory language function.--The treatment approach recommended by Wepman, and endorsed by others such as Longerich and Bordeaux,2 is to employ other sensory inputs in combination with auditory stimulation. Wepman notes In this area of disturbance more than any other must the total available resources of the patient be utilized. Where auditory stimuli are meaningless, the patient must be approached from the tactile-auditory, the visual-auditory, the kinetic-auditory and the com- bined visuo-tactile-kinetic-auditory. It should be noted that in each of the proposed approaches the audi- tory function is an integrated part of the training. The end product is not an ability to develOp a substi- tute for hearing, nor by circuitous methods to hope for a transfer of training, but to redevelop in the patient his use of the disturbed auditory function.3 lIbid., p. 199. 2Mary C. Longerich and Jean Bordeaux, Aphasia Therapeu- tics (New York: Macmillan Co., 1954), pp. 17-18. 3Wepman, Recovery from Aphasia, p. 199. -35- In terms of this objective, increasing the duration of the auditory stimuli was viewed as an attempt to accelerate the redevelopment of the disturbed auditory function by making the auditory stimuli more immediately usable to the patient. Schuell has maintained that auditory stimulation is the foundation of all aphasia therapy and is essential for all patients.1 She continues to stress the importance of auditory stimulation, stating in 1965 All evidence suggests that auditory stimulation is crucial in control of language processes. However, since feedback from more than one sensory modality may contribute to behavior, there is no reason for using this mode exclusively. This suggests that the first principle of treatment for aphasia should be the use of intensive auditory stimulation, although not neces- sarily stimulation through auditory channels alone.2 Nature of aphasic auditory language dysfunction.-- In addition to intensifying the stimulus appropriately, Schuell has discussed making the stimulus as adequate as possible for the patient. Many clinicians have heard patients make statements like those which Schuell referred to when she wrote lHildred Schuell, "Auditory Impairment in Aphasia: Significance and Retraining Techniques," JSHD, XVIII (1953), pp. 14-21. Hildred Schuell, Virginia Carroll and Barbara Stansell, "Clinical Treatment of Aphasia," JSHD, XX, (1955), pp. 43-53. 2Schuell, Jenkins, Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 338. -36- People talk too fast and say too much at a time for the aphasic patient to follow. Patients sometimes tell you that people do not seem to be talking right. To some patients it often seems they (peOple) are not even talking a language he knows. Luria has made a comment which seemed relevant to this type of aphasic experience. Discussing the role of the “auditory analyzer" in language and speech comprehension, he has written It is clear, therefore, that the perception of speech by hearing requires not merely delicate but systematized, hearing. When this selection of the essential, phonemic signs is no longer possible, phonemic hearing is dis- turbed. This is why the boundary between hearing speech and understanding it loses its Sharp distinction. A person ignorant of a foreign language not only does not understand it, but does not even hear it, i.e., he does not distinguish from the flow of sounds the articulated elements of the language and does not sys- tematize the sounds of speech according to its laws. The unfamiliar language is thus perceived by that per- son as a stream of unarticulated sounds, not only im- possible to understand, but inaccessible for accurate auditory analysis.2 To the clinician aware of the problems confronting the aphasic patient, this statement must have a familiar ring. Schuell discussed ways of manipulating the speech stimuli themselves so as to intensify or improve their ade- quacy for the patient. The objective was to enhance the patient's linguistic perception of the language units. The suggested variables were as follows: the loudness of the stimulus; the meaningfulness of the language units; lIbid., p. 339. 2Luria, Higher Cortical Functions, p. 102. -37- the length of the stimulus; and the duration of the stim- ulus.l Basing the tasks of this investigation on the theoretical notions of Schuell and her associates, it seemed logical to consider these variables in the design of the investigation. The four variables are given close considera- tion in the following section. Experimental Tasks and Associated Variables In this section the discussion centers on the rationale for selecting the two language tasks employed in this study and the variables considered in structuring the tasks. Selecting experimental taskS.--The tasks of word recognition and serial retention were employed in this study as procedures for operationalizing the two primary characteristics of aphasia identified by Schuell and Jenkins.2 They believed the language deficit in aphasia is characterized primarily by a reduction in available vocabulary and an impairment of verbal retention span. Impaired perception and impaired production of messages lSchuell, Jenkins, Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, pp. 339-40. 2Ibid., p. 113. -38- are additional characteristics which they felt may be secondary to the two primary characteristics. These char- acteristics of the language deficit are to be found in all language modalities to some extent and may be further im- paired by primary disturbances in auditory, visual and sensorimotor processes. To assess available vocabulary in the auditory modality, the word recognition task used seemed appropriate. Expecting the patient to point to a picture having the most meaningful association with a word spoken by the examiner is a task involving auditory recognition and the gross motor performance of pointing. Auditory recognition for single words might be considered a very low level test of a sub- ject's available vocabulary. Successful performance on this task indicates a capacity to link the auditory repre— sentation of the referent with a visual representation of the referent. The recognition of stimulus equivalence attested to the availability of the word meaning in the subject's receptive vocabulary.l In the second task it was necessary for the subject to retain a series of auditorily presented words, and then (maintaining the order of presentation) identify the pic- tures which had the most meaningful association with the words that had been presented. The period of retention lIbid.. pp. 149-53. -39- being on the order of from two to five seconds before the subject could begin his response, this task constituted a minimal test of auditory retention span. However, ex- pecting the subject's reproduction (by pointing) to reflect the order of the auditory stimuli made this a more complex task than it would have been were order not required. It also reduced the likelihood of a subject's guessing a cor- rect reSponse. The question of the relationship of performance on these tasks to language behavior was answered from the statistical analysis of the tasks comprising the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia. The Minnesota Test included tasks Similar to the two tasks employed in this study. In the factor analysis of the battery of tasks, the word recognition task and the serial retention task loaded high on the factor identified as a general dimen- sion of language behavior.1 The retention task, loading highest on this factor, had its second highest loading on a factor which appeared to represent "visual discrimination, and ensuing recognition and recall of learned visual pat- terns."2 The factor on which the word recognition task had its highest loading was a factor involving recognition of lIbid.. pp. 137-142. 2Ibid., p. 143. -4o- stimulus equivalence.l Aspects of the factor Of general language dimension were identified as: vocabulary, audi- tory retention span, and the ability to deal with language formulations at various levels of complexity.2 Therefore, it seemed the two tasks employed in this study were sampling language as it has been identified by these investigators. The linguistic perception of auditory stimuli by adult aphasics is said to be influenced by four factors.3 The following is a consideration of these factors. Loudness of the stimulus.--In addition to being alert to the incidence of moderate to severe hearing losses in aphasic patients,4 it has been suggested there may be an Optimal loudness level for maximum auditory language reception. Except in cases of clearcut perceptual prob- lems, most patients prefer listening at ordinary conver- sational levels in a free-field.5 Meaningfulness of the stimuluS.--It is well estab- lished that meaningful units of language are recognized lIbid., p. 151. 2Ibid., p. 153. 3Ibid., pp. 338—43. 4Barbara Stansell Street, "Hearing Loss in Aphasia," JSHD, XXII (1957). pp. 60-67. 5Schuell, Jenkins, Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 340. -41- more easily than non-meaningful patterns. In the sense that all stimuli used in this study were words in the native language of the subjects, all the stimuli were meaningful. On the word recognition task, an attempt was made to vary systematically the factor of meaningfulness in the stimulus words Of the task. A measure of meaningfulness used in several studies of both normal and aphasic language performance is the frequency with which a given word is used.1 Aphasic subjects have tended to follow the normal usage patterns but at reduced levels. This indicated that such a measure of meaningfulness was appropriate for the language performance of aphasic adults. The index of usage has generally been determined from word counts of written language,2 not spoken language. Another index of meaningfulness was suggested by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test.3 As empirically de- rived, the test presented a set of words scaled in terms of lJames Deese, "Form Class and the Determinants of Association," Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, I (1962), pp. 70-84. Joseph M. Wepman, pt 31,, "Psycholin- guistic Study of Aphasia: A Revision of the Concept of Anomia," JSHD, XXI (1956), pp. 468-77. Hildred Schuell, James Jenkins and Lydia Landis, "Relationship between Audi- tory Comprehension and Word Frequency in Aphasia," JSHR, IV (1961). pp. 30-36. 2Edward L. Thorndike and I. Lorge, Teachers Word Book of 30,000 Words (New York: Bureau of Publications, Columbia University, 1944). 3Lloyd Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: EXpanded Manual (Minneapolis: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1966), pp. 25-33. -42- the chronological develOpment of hearing vocabulary. Since, on this task of auditory recognition of words, the factor of meaningfulness was to be varied systematically, this scaled set of words seemed uniquely suited to assessing auditory receptive vocabulary in relation to the other independent variables of the study. An inspection of the stimulus words of the Peabody Test, in terms of their frequency of usage in written English, revealed those words associated with the period of early auditory language development were very common or highly frequent words in written English. Words associated with later periods of auditory language development were less and less frequent, becoming highly infrequent at the upper levels of the test. As Schuell, Jenkins, and Jimenez- Pabon have stated Both from a linguistic and neurophysiological viewpoint, we would expect that more would happen in the brain in response to frequently used words . . . than in response to rarely used words. . . .1 On a retention task, since the objective would be to equate the stimuli in terms of meaningfulness, only very common or highly frequent words would be used (e.g., the AA and A words of the Thorndike-Lorge General Count). In addition, on such a task, only one class or form of word might be used, perhaps the most frequent class of words. lSchuell, Jenkins, Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 399. -43- Recognizing the semantic-syntactic problems connected with classifying words, it appears that nouns are the most fre- quently occurring class Of words in the language. Length of the stimulus.--Controlling this variable is, reportedly, related to the reduced auditory retention span always found in aphasia.2 Control has been suggested by varying the number of syllables per word and the number of words in the stimulus. In a retention task, the number Of words in the stimuli could be varied systematically to create a range of complexity in the task. The discrete words from which the stimuli were created could be controlled in terms of the number of syllables per word. Duration of the stimulus.--In differentiating this factor from the factor of stimulus length, duration of the stimulus refers to the amount of time given to the utter- ance of elements in the stimulus, whereas stimulus length refers to the number of elements in the stimulus. Schuell, Jenkins and Jimenez-Pabon have found . . . patients with perceptual problems are often able to respond more adequately when a word or phrase is spoken a little more slowly than in ordinary conver- sational speech. However, inflection Should be natural, lRoger Brown, Words and Things (Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press, 1959), pp. 53-55. 2Schuell, Jenkins, Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 340. -44- and slowing should not fragment or distort the language unit. Like increasing loudness, slowing down the rate seems to help severely impaired patients initially, then to make no difference. The above statement implied that the effectiveness of increasing the duration of the stimulus is related to the severity of the auditory language disturbance. Hence, any test of the effectiveness Of durational increase Should control for this aspect of the language disorder. Control over the severity of auditory language deficit might be achieved through use of a section of the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia.2 One study has been found that applied this variable of stimulus duration (as defined here) to the comprehen- sion of language. This investigation, by Luterman, Welsh and Melrose,3 was designed to determine the effect Of stimulus duration on the performance of subjects in respond- ing to recorded phonetically balanced words. The experi- mental group consisted Of aged subjects with sensorineural hearing losses. The experimental conditions included con- ditions in which the stimulus words were increased in duration lIbid., pp. 340-41. 2Hildred Schuell, Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia: Administrative Manual (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 1965). 3David M. Luterman, Oliver L. Welsh and Jay.Melrose, "Response Of Aged Males to Time-Altered Speech Stimuli," JSHR. Ix (1966). pp. 226-30. -45- by a factor of 110% and by a factor of 120%. Other condi- tions were 10% and 20% time compression of the word dura- tions, as well as the original duration Of the stimulus words. The experimental group did not Show improved scores under the conditions of increased stimulus duration. The investigators found this outcome surprising, since they had observed, clinically, some advantage in Speaking slowly and clearly with aged subjects while engaged with them in Speech audiometry. While investigations of the relationship between rate Of speaking (words per minute) and intelligibility or listener preference have been conducted, the study cited above was the only relevant investigation employing the type of increase in stimulus duration used in this study. The relationship between Speech compression (decreasing stimulus durations) and comprehension Of messages has been investigated with normal subjects and with blind subjects.1 Increasing the duration of the auditory stimuli.2-- Descriptions of two processes for accomplishing the kind 1David B. Orr, Herbert L. Friedman and Jane C. C. Williams, "Trainability Of Listening Comprehension of Speeded Discourse," Journal of Educational Psychology. LVI (1965), pp. 148-156. Emerson Foulke, gpupl., "The Comprehension Of Rapid Speech by the Blind," Journal Of Exceptional Child- ;pp, xxrx (1962). pp. 134-141. 2Since increases in the duration of the stimuli was the experimental variable of interest in this study, and because the process by which the durational changes -46- of durational changes Of interest in this study have been published. A computer process for compressing and expanding the durational aSpects of speech has been reported, but the use of this process was not economically feasible at this time.1 In 1954, Fairbanks, Everitt and Jaeger2 pub- lished a description of an electromechanical apparatus capable of producing expanded or compressed versions Of tape recorded materials. It was an electromechanical device of this nature that was employed in the compressed speech and expanded Speech studies previously cited. This type of device was used in this study.3 The device operates on a sampling principle. In expanding a previously recorded signal, the device increases the temporal value Of segments of the acoustic patterns of the original signal and records a second time some portions were produced has had such very limited application, it seems appropriate to provide a general description of the process at this point and a more detailed description which appears in Appendix A. 1Emerson Foulke and Thomas Sticht, "A Review Of Research on Time Compressed Speech," (from the Proceedings of the Louisville Conference on Time Compressed Speech, University of Louisville, 1967). 2Grant Fairbanks, W. L. Everitt and R. P. Jaeger, "Method for Time on Frequency Compression-Expansion Of Speech" (1954), in Grant Fairbanks, Experimental Phonetics: Selected Writings (Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1966). Pp. 37-42. 3The device used is sold under the name of Eltro Information Rate Changer-Mark II by Infotronic Systems, Inc., 2 West 46 Street, N.Y. 10036. -47- of the signal. These extra portions (or second samples) appear in the expanded reproduction of the original signal immediately following and adjacent to the first reproduction of the segment. With these "extra samples" of the initial signal added and the temporal value Of all samples increased, the expanded reproduction of the signal will have a greater (expanded) duration than the original recording of the signal. When that original signal is speech, the expansion process increases durations of the phonemes in the word, as it reproduces some portions of the initial recording for a second time. This durational increase is accomplished without changing the frequency characteristics Of the ori- ginal signal in any perceptually significant way. This expanded reproduction can be recorded in the standard manner. Depending on the level Of expansion, the word, when expanded, will be perceived auditorily as being “stretched out" in comparison to the original version Of the word, free Of the pitch distortions associated with "slow played" speech which severely reduces its intelligibility.l At the upper limits Of the device there is a distracting type Of dis- tortion in the expanded reproduction which is a function of the temporal values of the repeated segments.2 The 1William R. Tiffany and Delmond Bennett, "Intel- ligibility Of Slow Played Speech," JSHD, IV (1961), pp. 248-258. 2Fairbanks, Everitt, Jaeger, "Compression-Expansion of Speech," p. 41. -43- upper limit of the device used in this study is 200% expan- sion (a reproduction having twice the duration of the ori- ginal signal). There is some indication this distortion has little influence on intelligibility with normal (non- aphasic) listeners.l Summary This review has indicated that as the study of aphasia has develOped during the last one hundred years, it became apparent that the language dysfunction experienced by the aphasic adult involved both the expressive and recep- tive modalities. It was possible to discern a trend toward conceptualizing the language dysfunction as a unidimensional characteristic, that was operationalized most meaningfully on the basis of observable language performance. It appeared these performances could be analyzed from a linguistic frame of reference. Also, it was possible to take a des- criptive approach, based on Observations of the aphasic adult's performance of language tasks. There appeared to be rather authoritative agreement that disturbances in auditory language reception are gener- ally a significant aspect of the total aphasic problem, and are, therefore, very important in the treatment of the 1George H. Kurtzrock, "The Effects of Time and Frequency Distortion upon Word Intelligibility," (Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Illinois, 1956). -49- 1anguage dysfunction. In View of this importance, it appeared worthwhile to investigate a means of facilitating auditory language function in aphasic adults. The proce- dure selected for investigation was increasing the duration of the auditory stimulus. There seemed to be some neurophysiological support for investigating this dimension of a linguistic stimulus. Several current theories of cerebral function offered a basis for assuming increased stimulus duration would posi- tively influence the performance of aphasic adults on cer- tain language tasks. In the clinical application of this procedure, several variables have been described that bear on the auditory language reception of aphasic adults. Two of these variables were the subjects' level of auditory lan- guage function and the meaningfulness of the stimulus material. These factors were considered in the design of the present experimental investigation. Two tasks that appeared to have a high degree Of relevance to a general dimension Of language dysfunction in aphasia were word recognition and retention span. There- fore, auditory tasks involving these capabilities seemed apprOpriate tasks upon which to test the influence of in- creased stimulus durations. An electromechanical means of producing temporal expansions in recorded words was available and appeared adequate for an initial experimental -5o- investigation of increased stimulus durations on the lan- guage receptivity of aphasic adults. CHAPTER III EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES This chapter includes a description of the devel- Opment of the stimulus materials for the word recognition task and the serial retention span task, as well as a dis- cussion of the recording procedures and the manner in which the durational increases were affected in these recordings. The chapter also deals with the selection and grouping of subjects and the procedures followed in presenting the tasks to the subjects. A description of the scoring pro— cedure is given. Selection and Preparation of Stimulus Materials Word recognition task materials.--The two forms Of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT)l were used as the source of stimulus words and reSponse pictures for the word recognition task. Each form of the test, consist- ing of 150 words each, was divided into two lists of seventy- five words each. The division was made by assigning the 1Lloyd Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Expanded Manual, (Minneapolis: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1965). -51- -52- Odd numbered items Of the form to list number 1 and the even numbered items to list number 2. Dividing both Form A and Form B of the test in this manner yielded four lists Of seventy-five words each, A 2' B 1' and B . 2 (See Table 1) that were designated as A 1’ TABLE 1.--Separation of PPVT Forms into Four Lists and Three Hearing Vocabulary Levels. PPVT - Form A PPVT - Form B HEARING VOCABULARY LEVELS II III List A1 List A2 List Bl List B2 odd items 1 - 50 1 even items 1 - 50 2 odd items 1 - 50 3 even items 1 - 50 4 Odd items 51 - 100 5 even items 51 - 100 6 odd items 51 - 100 7 even items 51 - 100 8 odd items 101 - 150 9 even items 101 - 150 10 odd items 101 - 150 11 even items 101 - 150 12 In terms Of hearing vocabulary, as defined earlier, it seemed reasonable to assume the overall difficulty of the four lists would be equivalent. With the two forms Of the PPVT being generally equivalent and dividing each form as described, this assumption seemed justified. Perry and Boswell report a correlation of .92 between the two forms -53- when used with aphasic adults.l Each form of the test was regarded as a scaled set of words reflecting the chronological development of hearing vocabulary through the first eighteen tO twenty years of life. It is the scaled aspect of the words in each form of the test that permitted the segmentation of each 75 word list into the three Hearing Vocabulary Levels indicated in Table l. The arbitrary division Of each list into three levels provided a means of looking at the available hearing vocabulary of adult aphasics in terms of three developmental levels of hearing vocabulary under the four experimental conditions. For the purposes of this study, the twenty-five words at Level I of each list were regarded as reflecting the initial phase of the early development of hearing vocabulary. The twenty-five words in the Level II, and Level III segments reflected intermediate and late phases Of hearing vocabulary development. For each of the stimulus words of the PPVT, there was a response plate consisting Of four pictures. There were 150 response plates, the same set of 150 plates being used with Form A of the test as were used with Form B. While the stimulus words differ for a given numbered item 1Peter S. Perry and Nathalie S. Boswell, "Adult Aphasic Performance on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test," (paper presented at American Speech and Hearing Association Convention, November 3, 1967). -54- on Form A and Form B, the response plate was the same. One of the four discrete pictures of each plate was the correct picture to associate with a stimulus word on Form A of the test, whereas another picture was the correct picture to associate with the same numbered stimulus word from Form B of the PPVT. The two other pictures were foils. Thirty-five millimeter slides were prepared Of the entire set of 150 response plates. These slides were used to project the response plates in front of the sub- jects, as will be described later. Serial retention span materials.--Two basic con- siderations entered into the develOpment Of materials for this task. They were building into the task an adequate range Of difficulty and maintaining equivalency of the discrete units of each stimulus. An adequate range of difficulty was built into this task in order to promote a reasonable range of scores. Only with such a range of scores would it be possible to observe the influence of the experimental treatment. For this study, the series contained two, three, four and five words. Secondly, it was important to keep the words used in the construction of all series equivalent on certain relevant criteria in order to stay as close to the defined purpose Of the task. The three equivalence criteria used in this study were grammatical class, frequency of usage -55- (written English), and hearing vocabulary level. One grammatical class of words was used. This class was nouns. Frequency of usage in written English was deter- 1 Schuell, mined from the Thorndike-Lorge General Count. Jenkins and Landis2 demonstrated the Significance of this variable on the ability of aphasic subjects to recognize words presented auditorily. All of the words used in the development of materials for this task had a frequency of usage equal to or greater than 100 times per million. This placed the words among the most frequently used written words of English. To establish equivalence in terms of hearing vocab- ulary, the empirically derived scaled word lists of the PPVT were again employed. For this retention span task, only objects pictured in the first fifty plates Of the PPVT were used, providing they met the frequency of usage criteria. By empirical determination, the names of the Objects pictured in the first fifty plates were within the mean expectation for individuals with a chronological 1Edward L. Thorndike, and I. Lorge. The Teacher's Word Book of 30,000 Words (New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College-Columbia university, 1944). 2Hildred Schuell, James J. Jenkins, and Lydia Landis, "Relationship Between Auditory Comprehension and Word Fre- quency in Aphasia," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research IV (1961), pp. 30-36. -56- age Of approximately five years.1 This provided some addi- tional assurance that the words used were a long standing part Of the subject's hearing vocabulary. It was determined that thirty words met these three criteria. Hence, there were thirty stimulus words and their associated pictures (from the PPVT) available for the develOpment of materials for this task. The thirty picture names are given in Appendix B. The arrangement of these pictures into response plates and the selection of the appropriate number of stimulus words for each plate was accomplished in the following manner. For this task, thirty-two response plates were required, one for each of the thirty—two stimuli. A series of discrete words was defined as a stimulus. The four duration conditions and the four complexity levels (two, three, four and five word series) created sixteen condi- tions. Providing two stimuli for each condition resulted in the need for thirty-two stimuli and associated response plates. The pictures for each of the eight-picture response plates were randomly selected, with replacement, from the pOOl of thirty available pictures. The thirty-two arrange- ments were photographed on thirty-five millimeter film and processed into slides. Two response plates were then assigned randomly to each of the Sixteen conditions. A lLloyd Dunn, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, p. 12. -57- plate was designated as either plate A or plate B for a particular cell, indicating the order in which they would be presented to subjects. With the assignment of plates to conditions com- pleted, the appropriate number of words for each stimulus was selected randomly from each response plate. The num- ber of words selected depended on the complexity level to which the response plate had been assigned. The auditory stimulus consisted of either two, three, four or five picture names with the word "and" inserted before the final word of the series. The inclusion of this conjunction made the stimulus a more complete unit, and enhanced its resemblance to a language unit. In Appendix B are found the names of the pictures included in each response plate and the stimulus associated with each response plate. The duration conditions to which the materials were assigned are also indicated in this appendix. The particular stimulus duration-stimulus complexity condition to which the materials were assigned was the same for all subjects. The discrete units of the stimuli were equivalent, and the different random order of conditions was employed for each subject. This randomization proce- dure will be elaborated in a later section. -58- Equipment and Materials The equipment and materials reported below were used in the phases of this experiment to be described in following sections of this chapter. Equipment. Tape recorders: Ampex, Model 350-2 Wollensak, Model T-1500 Sony, Model TC-211TS Microphone: Electro-Voice, Model 654 Spectographs: Voiceprint Laboratories Kay Sonograph, Model 606l-A Slide projector, rear projection: SonOSCOpe Amplifier-Speaker: Ampex 620 Power Level recorder: Bruel-Kjaer Level Recorder, Type 2305 Speech compresser-expander: Eltro Information Rate Changer-Mark II Materials. Kodacolor II, 35 mm. Scotch Brand Magnetic Recording Tape-111 _59- Recording of Auditory Stimuli One adult male recorded all the stimulus words for both the word recognition task and the serial retention Span task. The use of a Single voice eliminated the pos- sible influence of differences in the fundamental pitch of voices and variations in voice quality which would be asso— ciated with the use of more than one speaker. Equipment.--The speaker recorded the material in the sound isolated environment of a prefabricated sound treated booth, using a table mounted micrOphone. The recorder used was Operated at a tape Speed of fifteen inches per second. The speaker was able to monitor visually the loudness of his voice by means of the VU meter in the re- corder. The speaker was successful in keeping the intensity of the voice signal delivered to the recorder within plus or minus three decibels of the zero decibel reference on the VU meter. Intelligibility.-—An intelligibility check on the recording was made. The recording of the 300 single words and the thirty-two serial retention Span items was played to six persons who were not familiar with the Peabody Pic- ture Vocabulary Test. Each person listened to the recording by himself, responding to the recorded words by repeating each word heard. The responses of the six listeners ~60- revealed that all but five of the words in the original recording were correctly identified. These five words were re-recorded into the lists. The segments of the lists containing these words were then replayed to the six lis- teners. On the second presentation the five words were correctly identified, thus making it possible to say that the recorded words were 100% intelligible. Controlling duration of original stimuli.--It was important to exercise some degree of control over the dura- tion Of the words as they were uttered by the Speaker in the original recording of the stimulus material. It was felt the durations of the words in the original recording should conform to the expected durations of the words in a connected speech context. In the absence of any rele- vant previous research, this control was sought in the following manner. A random sample of one-syllable, two-syllable, three-syllable, four-syllable, and five-syllable words was drawn from the 300 words Of the PPVT. The words were drawn so that there were ten words in the first three cate- gories (one, two and three syllable words) and five words in the latter two categories (four and five syllable words). These forty words were then imbedded in syntactically cor- rect paragraph contexts. The Speaker rehearsed reading the several paragraphs until the rate of oral reading -6l— approximated the rate Of 160 words per minute. His oral reading of these paragraphs, at this rate, was then recorded. This rate Of 160 words per minute was an overall rate which includes pauses between sentences. This seemed to be an appropriate rate to use after examining the analysis of measures of speaking and oral reading rate presented by Johnson, Barley and Spriestersbach.l The duration of each Of these forty words was deter- mined from spectrograms Obtained for each of the words. In converting the linear measurements from the spectrograms to durations in milliseconds, one millimeter was given the value of 7.874 msec. The mean duration in milliseconds and the standard deviation was determined for each group of words, the one, two, three, four and five syllable word groups. Using the Obtained sample means (M) from each of the several groups Of words, an estimated range Of word durations was determined, such that 95% Of the words in that group would be expected to fall within this range. Based on the standard deviation (S.d.) of each sample, the formula used to derive the interval was 9 [M - 1.96 (S.d.) 3x 3M + 1.96 (S.d.)] = .95 lWendell Johnson, Frederic L. Barley, and Duane C. Spriestersbach, Diagnostic Methods in Speech Pathology, (New York: Harper and Row, Publishers, 1963), pp. 202-04. -62- Assuming the durations of words within each of these groups is nearly normally distributed, it seemed reasonable to say that additional Observations (x) of word durations, which fell within the interval for the word's particular group, were quite probably of a duration to be found in connected speech delivered at a rate of 160 words per minute. A word having a duration which fell outside the interval, for its appropriate group (one syllable, two syllable, etc.), would be regarded as an extremely unlikely event. The results Of this analysis of the forty words from context and the intervals computed for each group Of words is given in Appendix C. Treating the Obtained intervals as accep- tance intervals, it was possible to evaluate the recorded single word utterances and the words of the serial retention span stimuli in terms Of the extent to which their durations conformed to the durations one would expect in connected Speech. Rather than evaluate all of the recorded words, a random sample of words was drawn from both sets of recorded stimulus materials: thirty words from the 300 single word stimuli and six words from the thirty-two serial retention span stimuli. An g_priori decision rule was made. This rule was as follows: if more than 90% of the word dura- tions in the sample (thirty-six words) were within the acceptance intervals of their syllable groups, the entire set of recorded stimuli (single words and serial retention -53- items) would be accepted as having durations that would quite likely be found in connected Speech uttered at the rate of 160 words per minute. If 10% or more of the words in the sample (four or more words) had durations exceeding the limits of the appropriate interval, the entire set of words was to have been examined spectrographically, in terms Of their durations. Recorded stimuli having dura- tions falling outside the established acceptance intervals were to be re-recorded and made to conform to the derived intervals. The durational measures made on the randomly drawn sample of thirty-six words are given in Appendix C. It will be observed that only two of the thirty-six words had durations falling outside the acceptance interval for their class. The words were: transportation and illumination. They exceed the upper limits of their respective intervals by 261.58 msec. and 65.18 msec. Based upon the p priori decision rule, the dura- tions of all the recorded words were accepted as being within the expected durational limits for words in connected Speech uttered at 160 words per minute. In the sample Of thirty-six words, over 91% of the words had durations fall- ing within their appropriate acceptance interval. Calibration check tone.--TO have a means Of mea- suring the precision of the temporal expansions to be made -64- of the stimulus words, a puretone was recorded into the several lists of words. It was felt that a puretone pro- vided a well defined unit upon which to base these measure- ments. Its onset and termination were better defined than the beginning and ending of a word. Comparing expanded durations of the puretone with the original duration of the tone provided a measure of the durational changes pro- duced in the recorded words by the expansion process. A 1000 Hz tone was placed at the beginning and end of each of the four lists of seventy-five words, lists Al, A2, B and B2. The tone was also placed at the 1. beginning of the eight stimuli associated with each Of the duration conditions for the serial retention span task. The tone had a duration of approximately two seconds. The outcome of the calibration check is reported in the discussion which follows regarding the expansion of the recorded materials. Production of increased duration stimuli.--The increased durations of the auditory stimuli - single words and word series - were produced by an electromechanical device, sometimes referred to as a speech compresser- expander. From the original tape recordings of these stimuli, this electromechanical device sampled the stimuli with a degree Of temporal expansion that approximated the desired level of durational increase. The product of this -65- processing was reproduced on a second tape at 7.5 inches per second. As has been previously explained, given the normal duration of the signal or message, the device was able to increase the duration of the signal by a factor Of from 1.00 to 2.00. In other words, if the signal at the initial recorded duration (normal) was expanded by a factor of 1.50, it had a duration 50% longer than its initial duration. The signal was now 150% of its original time or duration. The fixed levels Of increased duration selected for this investigation were 125%, 150% and 175%. Adding the normal duration (duration as initially recorded or the 100% level) to the three increased durations provided four levels of the experimental treatment. These levels were selected as representative levels within the expansion capabilities of the electromechanical device. As previously mentioned, a calibration check tone was used to evaluate the precision of the expansion process. A power level recorder was used to obtain a read-out of the duration of the two second tone (1000 Hz) at the normal duration and at the three expanded durations. Appendix D contains the measurements Obtained from this calibration check. It will be Observed in Appendix D that each of the B four listS--A l and B2--were expanded to the three 1! A20 nominal duration levels. The calibration check tone appeared -66- at the beginning and end of each list. Hence, it was pos- sible to examine the precision of the processing of each list and the constancy of the processing throughout each list. The measurement of the reference tone (two seconds) was 61 mm., Obtained from the read-out of the level recorder. The measured durations (in milliseconds) for the expanded versions of the 1000 Hz tone were compared to this 61 mm. reference. The ratio Of the expanded duration to the refer- ence duration was expressed as a percentage Of the reference tone. The measurements and percentages are reported in Appendix D. All obtained expanSions were within five percent Of the nominal levels of durational increase specified. On the basis of an p_priori decision, and using the obtained measurements of the calibration check tone, the recorded word stimuli were accepted as being increased in duration to each of the three nominal levels. Before processing, it was decided that deviations of up to five percent from the specified levels of expansion would be accepted under that nominal level. Each of the four lists--Al, A2, 31' and B2--was now available at each of the four durations of 100%, 125%, 150% and 175%. 'Within each version of the four lists, groups of twenty-five words, representing each Of the three Hearing Vocabulary Levels were re-recorded, at 3.75 cps -67- on a separate tape. Separately storing each group of twenty- five words at each of the durational levels enabled the type of random presentation procedure to be described in a following section. Selection and Grouping of Subjects This section describes the manner in which subjects were identified and selected for participation in the study, as well as the procedure followed in separating the twenty- four subjects into two groups. The subjects were Obtained from five treatment centers in Michigan: Constance Brown Hearing and Speech Center, Kalamazoo; Veterans Administration HOSpital, Battle Creek; American Legion HOSpital, Battle Creek; Rehabilitation Medical Center, Lansing; and Speech Clinic-University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. DiagnOSiS.--Each of the subjects in this study had been diagnosed as having some degree of language deficit consequent to a cortical insult of some nature. For all subjects, the aphasia was exhibited at least ninety days following the cortical insult. Delaying the diagnosis until ninety days after the cortical insult generally avoids the period of greatest physiological instability and avoids transient language disturbances that are frequently asso- ciated with the immediate post cerebral insult period. -68- Schuell, Jenkins, and Jimenez-Pabonl have indicated the consensus is that most patients reach this stability in about ninety days. The diagnoses of aphasia were made by several pro- fessional Speech pathologists. Several examiners were involved as a consequence of having Obtained the subjects from several treatment centers. All of the examiners held at least a master's degree in Speech pathology and had three or more years of professional experience working with aphasic adults. The speech pathologists based their diagnoses, at least in part, on generally used diagnostic tests such as The Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia (MTDDA)2 and The Language Modalities Test for Aphasia.3 All Of the subjects had been accepted for treatment in language rehabilitation. This fact is cited as indicat- ing a degree Of physiological and psychological stability sufficient to coping with tasks such as those undertaken in this investigation. lSchuell, Jenkins, and Jimenez-Pabon, Aphasia in Adults, p. 160. 2Hildred Schuell, Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis ongphasia: Administrative Manual (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1965). 3Joseph M. Wepman and Lyle Y. Jones, The Language Modalities Test for Aphasia (Chicago: Education - Industry Service, 1961). —69- Auditory and visual screening.--It was desired that the auditory sensitivity and visual processes of all sub- jects be reasonably intact. Primary sensory deficits in these modalities could affect the subjects' performances on the experimental tasks in ways quite independent from the language deficit. Information was obtained on the auditory sensitivity of all subjects. This information was taken from recent threshold tests or from a screening assessment made at the time the subject was considered for participation in the experiment. The pure tone average, for 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz, by air conduction was no greater than 25 dB (ISO, 1964 Standard) for the better ear of each person aCcepted as a subject. In terms of visual function, the medical and clin- ical records of all potential subjects were examined for indications Of visual field problems or uncorrected visual acuity problems which would make the use of the response plates (in experimental tasks) difficult or impossible. Failing to find such evidence, the performance of each person on the~first two subtests of the Auditory Disturbance section of the MTDDA was scrutinized for suggestions of interfering visual disturbance. These subtests employ pictured objects in the response plates, as did the tasks Of the experiment. All of the persons accepted as subjects made correct responses on 74% or more Of the forty-two _70- items on these two subtests. In fact, the median percentage correct for all subjects on these two tasks was 99%. For subjects accustomed to wearing glasses, it was determined if the glasses enabled him to View the reSponse plates better. On the basis of these outcomes, it seemed reason- able to assume that all subjects were free from primary auditory sensitivity problems and primary visual problems which would impair their performance on the experimental tasks. Grouping subjects.--After determining that the adult aphasics had met the previously mentioned criteria, five additional subtests of the Auditory Disturbance Section Of the MTDDA were administered to them. These five subtests plus the two previously men- tioned subtests constituted the diagnostic tests Of this section and are intended to assess impairment in auditory discrimination, reduction of auditory comprehension of vocabulary, and reduction of auditory retention Span.l The two nondiagnostic subtests omitted from this section of the MTDDA were the test of understanding sentences and the test of understanding a paragraph. The subtests were lHildred Schuell, Differential Diagnosis Of Aphasia ggith the Minnesota Test (Minneapolis: University of Minne- sota Press, 1965), pp. 26-36. _71- administered as directed in the manual,1 with one slight modification. The reSponse plates for three subtests were presented to the subjects by means of the Slide projector to be described. This was done to condition the subject for the tasks of the experiment. This modification was made for the following subtests: recognizing common words; recognizing letters; and identifying items named serially. NO modification was made in the administration of the other four subtests concerned with discriminating between paired words, following directions, repeating digits, and repeating sentences. An error score was determined for each subject on the basis of the seven subtests, with the possible range of scores being zero to ninety-six. This score was regarded as a global measure of auditory language disturbance. Auditory language disturbance was a factor Of inter- est in analyzing the outcomes of this study, as indicated by the hypotheses stated earlier. To control and examine the influence of this variable on the performance of the experimental tasks by the subjects, the sample of twenty- four subjects was separated into two groups on the basis of these error scores. No attempt was made to control the range of audi- tory language disturbance scores by selectively accepting lHildred Schuell, Minnesota Test, pp. 5-8. -72_ subjects. The division of the total sample was made after all the subjects had been tested. Subjects with error scores in the lower 50% of the distribution of scores were designated as belonging to the group displaying low auditory language disturbance. Those subjects with error scores in the upper 50% of the distribution were designated as belonging to the high audi- tory language disturbance group. For the twenty-four sub- jects, the range of error scores was from four to sixty- five, with the median value being 16.5. As indicated in Table 2, the distribution of error scores for the low auditory language disturbance group differed from the distribution of scores for the high audi- tory language disturbance group. The statistical signifi- cance of this difference was tested by the Mann-Whitney TABLE 2. Comparison of Auditory Language Disturbance (ALD) Error Scores of Two Subject Groups. M Mann- Group Median Range Whitney U Low-ALD 10.5 4-16 0.00* High—ALD 32.0 17-65 — I - t *Probability (one-tail) < 0.00 -73- U Test,1 as the distributions of scores could not be con- sidered normal. The obtained U was 0. This U had a prob- ability Of approximately .00 in testing the hypothesis of no difference between groups against the directional hypoth- esis that the error scores of the high auditory language disturbance group were in fact higher than the error scores of the low group. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected in favor of the alternative hypothesis which stated the groups were different in terms of auditory language distur- bance. Other characteristics of the two groups that were examined and tested are given in Table 3. It would have been desirable to have found the two groups similar in terms of these characteristics. The characteristics were quantified in the following manner: age in years to nearest birthday; education in years; and time since onset of aphasia in months. Because the distributions of these measures could not be assumed to be normal, the Mann-Whitney U Test2 was used to test whether the two groups had been drawn from the same population. Since differences in either direction between the two groups was of interest, a two-tailed test for statistical significance was made, setting the confidence level at .95. lSidney Seigel, Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences (New YOrk: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1956), pp. 111-116. 21bid. -74- TABLE 3. Comparison Of Age, Educational Level, and Elapsed Time since Onset of Aphasia of Two Subject Groups (Low-ALD and High-ALD). Low-ALD High-ALD Mann- Characteristic Group Group Whitney U Age (years) Median 41 56 28.0* Range 21-60 37-72 Education (years) Median 13 12 42.0 Range 12-24 7-17 Onset (months) Median 14 23.5 53.5 Range 3-33 2-50 *Probability (two-tailed) < .05 The hypothesis of no difference between groups was rejected for the age characteristic, with °< = .05. Interest in the age of the subjects in the two groups stems from the question of the influence of aging on intelligence, more Specifically on vocabulary ability. As Wechslerl has indicated, vocabulary is less vulnerable to the influences associated with aging than the other capacities assessed by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Wechsler indicated little change in the vocabulary scores over the sixteen year period represented in the median age difference between lDavid Wechsler, The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence (Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins Co., 1958). p. 85. -75- the two groups in this investigation. The vocabulary test of the WAIS is more difficult than the PPVT, in that, on the WAIS the subject must define the word verbally. While the desirability Of having greater similarity in the ages of the two groups cannot be denied, the difference was not believed to be critical in terms of the experimental tasks of this investigation. Educational level showed no statistically signifi- cant difference between groups, and only one year Of real difference. The median educational level for the low audi- tory disturbance group was one year beyond high school, while the median level for the other group was high school graduation. This Similarity between groups was a most important factor. Lacking information concerning the pre- morbid intelligence of the subjects, it indicated some degree of equality between groups in terms of premorbid academic achievement. From this similarity, some degree of equality in premorbid vocabulary development was inferred.1 The time elapsed since onset Of the aphasia was the other group characteristic reported in Table III. On the basis of the statistical test, it was assumed the Observed difference was not significant. This factor was seen as reflecting on the degree of motivation subjects had toward participation in the experimental tasks. lIbid., p. 84. -75- Subjects with long standing aphasic conditions might be less motivated to participate in the experimental tasks than an equally impaired subject who had experienced the aphasia more recently. Presentation of Stimuli to Subjects Apparatus.--The tapes bearing the recorded audi- tory stimuli were played on a tape recorder that had a built-in photo-synchronizer circuit, enabling one to syn- chronize the audio output of the tape recorder with visual stimuli presented on a slide projector. The synchronizing signal from the recorder was delivered to a rear-projection type Of slide projection device. The image was projected on a glass screen at the front of the unit. Since the projector itself was behind the screen, touching the screen did not disturb the projected image on the screen. The projector was equipped to handle thirty-five millimeter Slides. The projected image from the slides of the Peabody Test response plates was practically the same size as that of the response plates themselves (6.25 inches x 8.75 inches vs. 6.25 inches x 8.50 inches). The image of the specially constructed response plates for the serial retention span task measured 9.25 inches x 6 inches. The inaudible signal, which activated the slide -77_ changing mechanism of the projector, was placed ahead of the stimuli on the tapes. The signal preceded the stimuli by a period of time sufficient to allow the slide changing mechanism to get the slide into position before its asso- ciated word was played. The slide was in position anywhere from two to three seconds before the auditory stimulus was played. The auditory stimuli were played through an amplifier- speaker rather than through the internal Speaker of the tape recorder. The amplifier-speaker was used to provide an auditory stimulus having better fidelity than would have been Obtained from the internal Speaker of the tape recorder. The amplifier-speaker was placed directly above the pro- jection screen. Hence, the auditory stimuli and the response plate were aligned in the same vertical plane. The output of the speaker was adjusted to a comfortable loudness level, as determined by questioning the subject about the loudness of the signal. In general the level of the auditory stimuli was on the order of 60 to 70 dB SPL (re: .0002 dynes cmz) at the ear of the subject. Test conditions.--The conditions under which the testing was done varied as a result of having secured sub- jects from several different treatment centers. Each sub- ject was tested in the room (or a room quite similar to the room) in which he had previously received therapy. The -78- rooms were reasonably free of visual and auditory distrac- tions and were physically comfortable in terms Of temperature and ventilation. Also, these conditions were essentially constant for each subject for each Of the three test ses- sions required for the completion of the tasks in the experiment. Each of the three test sessions lasted from thirty to sixty minutes. The first session approached sixty minutes for all subjects since the Auditory Disturbance Section of the Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia was administered during this session. Testing of a sub- ject was completed within a three day period, with the elapsed time between test sessions being twenty-four hours. The subjects were seated in front of the projection screen and were assisted in positioning themselves at a comfortable visual distance and angle in relation to the screen. For no subject did this position preclude his being able to touch easily and comfortably the screen of the projector. If the subject was accustomed to wearing glasses, it was determined whether or not his glasses enabled him better to view the visual image on the screen of the projector. The experimenter was seated to the left of the subject. From this position, the experimenter was able to observe the subjects' pointing responses and still con- trol the tape recorder and record the subjects' responses to the auditory stimuli. -79- Randomization of stimuli.--The two sets Of stimuli-- single words and serial items--were randomized independently, with the randomization of the stimuli for each of the tasks being different for each subject. The randomization pro- cedure for the word recognition task is described first, followed by a description of the procedures employed in randomizing the serial retention items. The twenty-four randomizations, used in presenting the stimuli for the word recognition task to the subjects, were develOped by first assigning each Of the four lists of seventy-five words, lists Al’ A2, B1' and B2, to one of the four stimulus duration conditions. The assignment was made so as to employ all twenty-four permutations Of the four lists. In each of these twenty-four arrangements, the first list was assigned to the 100% stimulus duration; the second list to the 125%.duration; the third tO the 150% duration; and the fourth list to the 175% stimulus duration. Within each of these lists, the three Hearing Vocabulary Levels were represented. At each level there were twenty- five stimulus words. One such matrix is given in Table 4. Each block of the matrix represents twenty-five stimulus words. In Appendix E is found the arrangement of lists for each subject. For each of the twenty-four arrangements Of the lists, the blocks of twenty-five words were indexed in the same manner. Based on this system of indexing, the -80- TABLE 4.--Sample Arrangement of Lists with Block Index Numbers. Lists H H no u: .p Hearing Vocabulary Level II 5 6 7 8 III 9 10 11 12 1.00 1.25 1.50 1.75 Stimulus Duration Conditions order of presentation for the twelve groups of twenty-five words was randomized for each subject. There were twenty- four such randomly selected block sequences. The serial retention span stimuli were randomized in a manner similar to the randomization of blocks for the word recognition stimuli. For the serial retention stimuli, each of the four stimulus durations were coupled with four levels of stimulus complexity (two, three, four, and five word series). This resulted in a sixteen block matrix. The auditory stimuli and their associated response plates were not varied across durations for this task, as they were for the word recognition task. Each block contained two serial retention items, the number of words in the auditory stimulus being determined by the complexity level -81- at which the items were placed. The blocks were indexed (in the manner indicated for the word recognition stimuli) with the numbers one through sixteen. One randomization of the sixteen blocks was generated for each subject, for a total of twenty-four block sequences. One randomization for the word recognition task and for the serial retention Span task were paired, and assigned a subject number. The subject was exposed to both of the experimental tasks at each of the three test ses- sions: four sets of single words with each set containing twenty-five words; and five or six pairs of serial items. The order in which the tasks were presented to a subject varied systematically from test session to test session. If word recognition was the first task of session one, it was the second task Of session two, and the first task of session three. The word recognition task was the first task Of session one for half of the subjects, while the other half Of the subjects began session one with the serial retention span task. This arrangement of tasks within sessions Offset possible order effects of one task upon the other. Instructions to supjects.-—Following the administra- tion of the Auditory disturbance section of the MTDDA, either the word recognition task or the serial retention Span task was administered to the subjects. At the first test session, the following instructions were given. -82— For the word recognition task, the instructions were as follows: I'm going to Show you some pictures like these (point- ing to screen with PPVT Example A diSplayed). You see there are four pictures (pointing to each picture). You will hear a word, then I want you to point to the picture which best tells the meaning of the word. Let's try one. Point to the picture which best tells the meaning of crib. (Get correct response before proceeding.) Fine. (Show PPVT Example B on screen.) Show me IEB: (Get correct response.) Good. (Show PPVT Example C on Screen.) Butterfly. Good. Now each time you hear a word point to the picture which best tells the meaning of the word. You may not be sure of the meaning of some words. But look carefully at all the pictures anyway and point to the one you think is right. DO you have any questions? Here's the first one. Instructions for the serial retention Span task were as follows: You are going to see pictures like these (pointing to screen on which Sample eight-picture plate is dis- played). You see there are eight pictures (pointing to each picture). YOu will hear several words. There may be two words, or three words, or four words, or five words. The words will name things pictured here. I want you to point to the pictures named. Let's try one. Show me p£p_and knife. (Get correct response emphasizing order.) Good. Show me duck and‘ppn. (Get correct response emphasizing order.) Fine. Show me duck,‘pyg, and broom. Now each time you hear the words point to the pictures named in the same way you heard the words. Sometimes you may not be sure of all the words. But look carefully at all the pictures anyway and point to the pictures you think are right. Do you understand? Do you have any questions? Here's the first one. AS it was necessary, subjects were encouraged and directed to make the pointing response even though they had to make "a wild guess." A response was obtained from each subject to every stimulus. The subject was given as much time as he took to make his response. The subjects were -83- reminded of the nature of the tasks at the beginning of the second and third sessions. Scoring responses.--The experimenter recorded the subject's responses by writing down the number or numbers of the picture (or pictures) to which the subject had pointed. The response record sheet for the word recognition task contained the ordered set of stimulus words as well as the number of the correct picture for each stimulus word. The subject's score was the number of correct responses. The response record sheets for the serial retention span task provided a space in which the experimenter wrote the sequence of picture numbers pointed to by the subject. A response was scored as correct if, and only if, the sub- ject pointed to all the pictures named in the same order in which they were named. The subject's score on this task was weighted, depending upon the number of words in the stimulus. The scoring procedure for each correct response was as follows: two points for a two word series; three points for a three word series; four points for a four word series; and five points for a five word series. CHAPTER IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION This chapter presents the results obtained from the procedures described in Chapter III. Twenty-four aphasic adults participated in a word recognition task and a retention span task, in which the stimuli were pre- sented auditorily, under four experimental conditions of stimulus duration. On the word recognition task, the stimuli were associated with one of three levels of hearing vocabulary, as an index of the meaningfulness of the stim- uli. On both tasks, subjects were assigned to either the low auditory language disturbance group (hereafter referred to as LALD Group) or the high auditory language disturbance group (hereafter referred to as HALD Group) on the basis of their performance on the Auditory Disturbance Section of the Minnesota Test for the Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia.1 Subject performance was measured by the number of correct picture identifications made in response to the auditory presentation of either a single word (recognition lHildred Schuell, Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia: Administrative Manual (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 1965), pp. 5-8. -84- -85- task) or a series of from two to five words (retention task). The number of correct identifications per condition constituted the subject's score and was considered the de- pendent variable. For each Of the tasks, there were independent var- iables that were of interest in terms Of assessing the design Of the experiment. For the word recognition task, these independent variables were subject groups and levels of hearing vocabulary. The independent (control) variable for the retention task was subject groups. These variables are considered first in the following report of results. Of major interest, of course, was the influence of stimulus durations, the third independent variable. In addition to examining the independent influence of this variable on performance in both tasks, the interaction of stimulus durations with the other independent variables was of equal interest. . Assumptions underlying statistical tests.--Before proceeding with the several statistical analyses, it was necessary to consider the extent to which the assumptions underlying the use of the statistics had been met. Analysis of variance and a nonparametric correlation were the sta- tistics employed. The assumptions underlying the use of the analyses of variance were (1) the performance measure was normally -86- distributed in the aphasic adult pOpulation, (2) the popula- tions had variances which were homogeneous over the Observa- tions to be made, and (3) the performance made by each subject was independent of the performance of every other subject. Available studies of the performance of aphasic adults on tasks such as those employed in the present study indicated that the distribution of scores was moderately to markedly skewed.l The degree of skewedness appeared to depend largely on the range of aphasic disturbance repre- sented in the particular sample of the general pOpulation. Allowing that the characteristics may be moderately skewed in the aphasic adult population, it was recognized that the F-test is quite insensitive to the shape of the distribu- tion. Even when a good approximation to a normal distribu- tion fails, Lindquist has stated . . . the F-distribution seems so insensitive to the form of the distribution of criterion measures that it hardly seems worthwhile to apply any statistical test to the data to detect non-normality, even though such tests are available. Unless the departure from normality is so extreme that it can easily be detected by mere inspection Of the data, the departure from normality will probably have no appreciable effect on the validity of the F-test. . .2 1Peter S. Perry, Personal communication, November, 1967. Hildred Schuell, Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia with the Minnesota Test (Minneapolis: university of Minn- esota Press, 1965), pp. 27-29, 31. 2E. F. Lindquist, Design and Analysis Of Experiments in Psychology and Education (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1953), p. 87. -87- The above is conditional upon the distribution being homo- geneous in both form and variance for the treatment pOpula- tions. Regarding the assumption Of equal variances across the treatment groups, it is generally recognized that so long as the sample sizes are equal for all conditions of the analysis, violations Of the homogeneity assumptions have little effect upon the F-distribution.1 Sample sizes for all conditions of both analyses were equal. It seems, therefore, the assumption of normal dis- tribution and homogeneity of variance can be satisfied for this study. The third major assumption pertains tO the inde- pendence of the various observations. Only on the auditory language disturbance factor was independence true. The factors of stimulus durations and hearing vocabulary levels were not independent. Each of the twenty-four subjects was Observed under all twelve combinations of stimulus durations and hearing vocabulary levels. It was for this reason the three-factor analysis of variance for repeated measures on two factors was selected as the most appropriate statistic to test the null hypotheses derived from the questions concerning word recognition. A two-factor analysis lWilliam L. Hays, Statistics for Psychologists, (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1964), p. 408. Lindquist, Design and Analysis, pp. 33-34. -88- with repeated measures on one factor was selected for the retention task. In addition to the assumptions specified above, Winerl has listed two additional assumptions that are necessary to repeated measures designs. They are as fol- lows: the order in which the repeated measures are made must be randomized independently for each subject, and the p subjects in a group must be a random sample from a Spe- cified population. The randomization procedures described in the previous chapter clearly indicate that the first of these Special assumptions was satisfied. The second assumption was met in a satisfactory manner by drawing subjects from several clinics in Michigan. The Spearman rank correlation coefficient was used to assess the degree of association between a subject's performance on the word recognition task and his perform- ance on the retention Span task. The use Of this statistic requires that both variables me measured in at least an ordinal scale so that the individuals can be ranked in two ordered series.2 The ordinal level of measurement was achieved on both variables of interest in this comparison: 1B. J. Winer, Statistical Principles in Experimental Design (New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1962), pp. 298-301. 2Sidney Siegel, Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences (New York: McGraw-Hill Company, Inc., 1956). P. 202. -89- word recognition scores and weighted retention Span scores. NO further assumptions need be made in the use of this statistic. The presentation of the results of the statistical treatments is organized on the basis of the two tasks in which the subjects participated, followed by presentation of the inter-task comparisons. A discussion of the Obtained results concludes this chapter. Results from Word Recognition Task The null hypotheses generated from the three ques- tions concerning the performance of the subjects on the word recognition task were tested by means of three factor analysis of variance for repeated measures.1 The repeated measures entered the analysis as the factors of stimulus durations (Durations) and hearing vocabulary levels (HVL), the experimental variable and a control variable, respec- tively. The second control variable was subject groups (Groups): the group of subjects with low auditory language disturbance (LALD); and the group with high auditory lan- guage disturbance (HALD). The three factors of the analysis were arranged as a three dimensional table of a 4 x 3 x 2 design. The four columns represented the four Durations, lWiner, Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, pp. 319-35. -90- while the three rows represented the three HVL. The third dimension was conceptualized as two slices of the 4 x 3 matrix (Durations x HVL), the slices representing the two subject groups--LALD Group and the HALD Group. The depen- dent variable (word recognition score) was entered, for each subject, in the appropriate cell of the twelve cell matrix associated with his subject Group. This analysis of variance was conducted on a Con- trol Data Corporation 3600 Digital Computer (as were all 1 The F-ratio was statistical computations in this study). used in testing the statistical significance of the variance attributable to the three main factors and the several interactions. The .05 level of confidence was established. The summary of this analysis is given in Table 5. Before considering the influence of the experimen- tal variable, an examination was made of the control var- iables, auditory language disturbance (Groups) and hearing vocabulary levels (HVL). The summary table shows the Obtained F-statistic for both Groups and HVL is statistically significant. For the Groups, the probability of the F-statistic was approximately 0.002 while for the HVL factor, the prob- ability was less than 0.0005.2 These outcomes provided 1Agricultural Experiment Station, "STAT Series Description NO. 14: Analysis of Variance with Equal Fre- quency in Each Cell," (East Lansing, Michigan State Univer- sity, 1968). 21bid. -91- Hm>ma mo. map Ocommn HGMUNMHGmHm# nmm www.mmmm- Hmpop mmo.m mm- omm.om¢ omm + OONN u N #nem.m omm.HH o «mm.as O x m x N ooh. moo.~ o mmm.m- m x a Hmm.6m mm Nev.mmam so + O u N #Nmm.-H w-o.¢¢- H s-o.S¢- -o- masons Hm-.v mo mmo.N-N mm + Oom u N #nmo.m vom.NH m vam.nm O x m o-m.o mm-.m m nem.-H Am- co-pmusa mm¢.mm 66 om~.-NHH ON + OON n N emo.m omw.mm m osm.wo- O x 4 #nmm.mo- N-A.wom¢ m ¢~6.moom AN- .>mq .pmoo> .pmmm oaumflumum mumswm mmumsvm .m Gmmz .mQ MO saw OUGMHHM> MO OOH-DOW coauflcmoomm OHOB on» so mGOHumusn mSHSEflum Ugm .xmma .mmsouw pomnnsm .mH0>0A mum Iasnmoo> mafiummm mo muommmm mnu mcflummEoo mosmflum> mo mama-NS4 m0 mumEESmll.m mqmde _92- general support for the design Of the experiment. Both factors were expected to exert an independent influence on performance of the task, and it was desired to control for their effect in examining the influence of stimulus dura- tions on word recognition. While the F-statistic indicated the two groups of subjects performed differently, it was noted that the LALD Group made the higher scores. The mean difference, averaged across all twelve duration x hearing vocabulary conditions, was 3.99. In other words, the group with better auditory language function made, on the average, about four more correct responses per treatment condition than did the group of subjects with poorer auditory language function. Since the F-statistic did not reveal where the significant differences in hearing vocabulary levels lay, a critical difference test1 was employed to determine if, in fact, the expected differences in hearing vocabulary levels did exist. On the basis of the rationale for struc- turing these levels, the scores on Level I should have been significantly greater than those at Level II, and Level II scores should have been greater than those Of Level III. In Table 6, the means at each of the hearing vocab- ulary levels for the two Subject Groups are given, as well lLindquist, Design and Analysis, p. 93. -93_ TABLE 6.--Hearing Vocabulary Level Means, and Differences between Means for Each Auditory Language Disturbance Group '1 I HALD LALD Group Group Means Level I 21.292 24.542 Level II 16.146 21.833 Level III 8.354 11.375 Difference between Means Levels I-II 5.146* 2.709 Levels II-III 7.792* 10.458* Levels I-III 12.938* 13.375* *critical difference, .05 level as the differences between means within groups. It was determined that a difference of more than 4.254 would be significant at the .05 level for differences within the subject groups. As seen in Table 6, one of the differences of interest was not statistically significant: the Level I - Level II differences for the LALD Group. With the ex- ception of this difference, all other differences were statistically significant. It may also be noted, in the analysis of variance summary table, that the interaction of Groups x HVL was not significant, statistically, reflect- ing the strength of the independent influence of these two factors on the performance of the word recognition task. The adequacy of the experimental design, with regard -94- to the control exercised over auditory language disturbance, appeared to be substantial. From this same vieWpOint, the control of hearing vocabulary appeared to have failed at only one point. Results of experimental treatment.--Turning to the results of major interest, the null hypotheses derived from the first three questions stated in Chapter I (p. 3) are stated and then evaluated in terms of the outcomes of the statistical analysis. The general question of the influence of stimulus durations on word recognition lead to the following null hypothesis: There is no difference in the mean improve- ment Of auditory recognition under several conditions of stimulus duration on a task involving auditory recognition of Single words. In the summary table (Table 5, page 91) the test of this null hypothesis was found in the main effect for Duration. This main effect was not statistically significant, making it impossible to reject the null hypothesis of no differ- ence from the variations produced in the duration of the stimuli. On the basis of this experiment, increasing the duration Of words did not appreciably affect the performance of aphasic adults in recognizing single words presented _95- auditorily. It was noted that the influence of Duration was tested here on the performance of all subjects, ignoring potential differences that may exist between subjects mani- festing different degrees of aphasic involvement, an inter- action to be considered later. The question of the interaction of stimulus dura- tion and hearing vocabulary levels generated the following null hypothesis: The duration of the stimulus words will not affect mean performance in recognizing single words presented auditorily at several levels of hearing vocabulary. The Duration x HVL interaction provided the means for test— ing this null hypothesis. The F-statistic resulted in a ratio that was not significant statistically. This indicated there was a systematic relationship between these two var- iables. This null hypothesis was not rejected, on the basis of this analysis. Of the two remaining interactions, both were statis- tically significant: Duration x Groups, and the triple interaction of HVL x Durations x Groups. The two inter- actions were of interest to the third question posed in this study. A first approach to an understanding of these inter- actions was a graphic approach.l lWiner, Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, pp. 178-84. -95- In utilizing this graphic approach, it seemed helpful to examine first an interaction found to be not significant statistically. The Groups x HVL interaction was re-examined for this purpose. The plot of this inter- action is given in Figure 1. In inspecting such plots of data (in this case, means are plotted), it was helpful to remember that inter- actions not statistically significant reflect a high degree of parallelism between related sets of observations. The plot of the Group x HVL interaction certainly reflected this characteristic. The plot of means for both groups (LALD Group and HALD Group) were quite parallel to each other and parallel to the plot of means for all subjects combined (Total Group). The relationship was quite system- atic across all levels of Hearing Vocabulary. As stated previously, this outcome added strength to the experimental design, attesting to the important independent influence of these two variables. To test the null hypothesis developed from the third question posed in this investigation, the statistical significance of the Groups x Duration interaction was con- sidered. The null hypothesis was: Under several conditions of stimulus duration, there will be no difference in the mean performance between subjects identified as having a low degree of audi- tory language disturbance and those -97- FIGURE l.--Graph of Group Means and Total Group Mean Number of Correct Responses - Word Recognition at Three HVL 25:: 24 -- 23 .r 22 -. 21-- K 20 -- \ 19" \ .. 18 u \ l7d- \ 1.-. x 15,, \ l4-m \ 13 +- \ 12 u \ 11 1F 10-. 9 “ 8-p /L 01’ 61—— LALD Group ‘ Total Group \‘ ~HALD Group I II III Hearing Vocabulary Levels -98- subjects identified as having a high degree of auditory language disturbance, on a task of auditory word recognition. The real significance of the Groups x Durations interaction was examined in Figure 2, at the heavy lines labelled A, B, and C. At line A, it was seen that the means, at each of the four durations, for the LALD Group arranged themselves in a manner approximating a straight line. The means of the HALD Group (B), on the other hand, did not make as good an approximation to a straight line. This departure of the HALD Group Means from the configuration of the LALD Group Means was sufficient to cause the Groups within Dura- tions interaction to reach statistical significance. Further- more, since the plot of means for the Total Group (C) re— flected a relatively straight line function, it was in the HALD Group (B) that departure from parallelism was to be found. It appeared that this departure was produced by the observed difference between means at 125% and 175% Stimulus Duration. When, in fact, the differences between means within each of the two groups was tested, the differ- ence between means at 125% and 175% in the HALD Group was the only difference found to be statistically significant at the 5% level of confidence. The test used was the critical difference procedure suggested by Lindquist.1 The lLindquist, Design and Analysis, pp. 164—66. 25 24 23 22 21 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 -99- FIGURE 2.--Graph of Means for Hearing Vocabulary Levels of Groups at Four Durations. \ \ @U‘J II 100% 125% 150% 175% Stimulus Durations :sIeAeq TE"I l dnoxo :sIaAaq duels 01v1-_. dnoxs 01VH=== IEQOJ, 000 0 V 8 suoTQEJna qe sueew IeAaq KJeInqeooA BUIIEBH -lOO- differences between means within each of the two groups of subjects are reported in Table 7. TABLE 7.--Mean performance for Each Auditory Lan- guage Disturbance Group at Four Stimulus Durations, and Within Group Mean Differ— ences between Duration Conditions W HALD LALD Group Group Duration Means 100% 15.139 19.472 125% 16.028 19.000 150% 15.389 19.111 175% 14.500 19.417 Difference between Means 125%. 100% 0.889 -0.472 150% 100% 0.250 -0.361 175% 100% -O.639 -0.055 150% 125% -O.639 0.111 175% 125% -1.528* 0.417 175% 15G% -0.889 0.417 *critical difference, .05 level. The difference in means at 175%P125%.in the HALD Group (the only statistically significant difference) was a negative difference. Performance was better at the dura- tion more closely approximating the "normal" duration of 100%, i.e., the 125% duration, than it was at the 175% duration. No real significance was attached to this finding since the performance at 175% duration was not critically different from the performance at 100% duration. The -101- statistical significance of this interaction might lead one to reject the null hypothesis under discussion. However, the interpretation made of this interaction lead to the decision to postpone judgment on this hypothesis. This decision was influenced by an inspection of the statistically significant triple interaction of HVL x Duration x Groups. This interaction was vividly displayed in Figure 2. While the influence of Durations was negli- gible, the control variables of Groups and HVL appeared to interact in a highly nonsystematic manner. At both the 125% and 150% Durations, it was possible to observe the between group interaction at IIfI, and IIfII. While con- tributing to the statistical significance of the triple interaction, these observations were expected, considering the basis upon which the subjects were grouped. That basis was degree of auditory language disturbance. Examining the within group differences by levels of hearing vocabulary at the four durations, it was seen that the LALD Group plot of means, at the three HVL, bore a strong resemblance to the LALD Group Means (A). Such was not the case when a similar examination was made within the HALD Group. It was easy to see the dissimilarity in the plot of means for the HALD Group at HVL II and HVL III. The means at these hearing vocabulary levels not only dif- fered from those at HVL I and HALD Group Means (B), but HVL II differed in its configuration from HVL III. -102- It was undoubtedly in both the between groups interaction and within the HALD Group itself that the sta— tistical significance of the triple interaction was gener- ated. The observed trends within the HALD Group at the three HVL, while not of statistical significance, presented an interesting pattern that is discussed in a later section of this chapter. The interest in this pattern was the basis for postponing a decision on the null hypothesis concerning the influence of the durations on the two groups of subjects. Results from Retention Span Task Two of the questions prompting this study concerned the performance of aphasic adults on a retention Span task in which the stimuli were presented auditorily at several levels of complexity. The null hypotheses developed from these questions were: (1) Increasing the duration of the speech stimuli will have no influence on the mean performance of a retention span task in which the words are presented auditorily and in series. (2) There is no difference in the mean performance of two groups of subjects, identified as having different amounts of auditory language distur- bance, at several levels of stimulus duration -103- on a task requiring the retention of words presented auditorily and in series. The performance of the subjects was analyzed by a two factor analysis of variance, with repeated measures on one factor.1 The repeated measure was on stimulus duration (Duration). The second factor was Groups, defined as for the word recognition task. The grouping of subjects was the same as for the word recognition task. The two factors were arranged in a two dimensional table of a 4 x 2 design. The four columns represented stimulus durations, while the two rows represented subject groups (Groups): as before, LALD Group and HALD Group. The dependent variable, weighted retention span score, was entered for each subject in the appropriate cell of the eight cell matrix. The F-ratio was used in testing the statistical significance of the variance attributable to the two main effects and the two way interaction. The .05 level of confidence was established. The summary of this analysis of variance is given in Table 8. In this summary table, the only statistically sig- nificant F-test obtained was that for Groups. The measured difference in performance between the two groups was 13.61; the mean for the LALD Group was 18.06, while the HALD Group lWiner, Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, pp. 302-09. .Hm>mH mo. may onoamn unmoflmflcmum# W mm mmv.oomn Hmuoa 1. _ mmm.>n mm mum.vaha um + o n N #mnm.om oop.a¢¢¢ H oms.H¢¢¢ Ame msouw moa.ma we 5mm.omm 04 + 0mm n N omw.o omm.m m mmo.oa m x 4 hm>.o mmo.mH m «Ha.mm Adv aofiumusn oHumHumum mumsvm mmumsqm m coo: ma mo 85m oUQMHHm> mo monsom l i .Mmma smmm coaucmumm may no mGOHumusn msasfiflum Ucm mmsouw uomflnsm wo muomwmm mnu mcflnmmeoo mocmflum> mo mammamcd mo mumsasmll.m mamas -105- had a mean of 4.45. This difference was in the expected direction. Since the main effect for Duration was not statis- tically significant, there was no basis for rejecting the first null hypothesis concerned with performance on the retention Span task. The second null hypothesis could not be rejected since the Duration x Groups interaction was not statistically significant. Under the conditions of this experiment, it must be reported that the duration of the stimulus did not have any important influence on the performance of this reten- tion span task by aphasic adults. Furthermore, the two groups of subjects did not perform differently at any of the four stimulus duration conditions. Results of Inter-Task Comparison The final question posed in this investigation dealt with the relationship of subjects' performances on the two tasks employed in the experiment. The question led to the following null hypothesis: There is no relationship between performance on a word recognition task and on a retention Span task in which the stimuli are presented auditorily. Since both analyses of variance had indicated that the -lO6- established grouping of subjects was a significant factor in the performance of both tasks, the measure of associa- tion between tasks was applied within groups. The Spearman rank correlation was the measure of association used.1 The significance of the correlations, as they might reflect on the aphasic adult population, was determined. If, in fact, the two performances were correlated in the population, it seemed reasonable to assume the correlation was positive. This reasoning was based on the previously cited theoretical 2 The .05 level of confidence notions of Jenkins and Schuell. was established. The obtained correlations are presented in Table 9. The only statistically significant correla— tions were obtained in the group having the poorest auditory language function, the HALD Group. TABLE 9.--Spearman Rank Correlations of Performance on Word Recognition Task and Retention Span Task for the Two Subject Groups. W Stimulus Durations Group_ 100% 125% 150% 175% LALD -0.456 -0.l44 -0.287 -0.101 HALD 0.599* 0.670* 0.359 0.506* *significant at .05 level (one-tail) lSiegel, Nonparametric Statistics for Behavioral Sciences, p. 202. 2Hildred Schuell and James J. Jenkins, "The Nature of the Language Deficit in Aphasia," Psychological Review LXVI (1959), pp. 50-51. -107- It was not possible to reject the null hypothesis at any of the duration conditions for this LALD Group. For the HALD Group, at the three durations, it was possible to reject the hypothesis of no association in favor of a hypothesis stating a positive relationship between perform- ance on the word recognition task and the retention span task. The durations were 100%, 125%, and 175% stimulus duration. Discussion Within its limitations, this investigation has demonstrated that increasing the duration of auditory stimuli does not significantly alter the performance of aphasic adults on two tasks which appeared to be highly relevant to the language dysfunction in aphasia. The recog- nition of words presented auditorily seemed to be related to the reduction in available vocabulary, while the reten- tion span task (for words presented auditorily in series) seemed to be clearly related to the degree of impairment in verbal retention span. It was entirely possible the increases in stimulus duration, employed in this study, were not great enough to reach an effective level of improvement for the aphasic subjects. This point deserved serious consideration. If Ibid. -108- the clinical modifications made in the duration of auditory language stimuli are, in and of themselves, an effective means of facilitating language reception, then the levels of durational increase may exceed the levels employed in this study. However, this possibility was not fully clari- fied by this study. It should be noted that this study was unable to provide information about the possible influence of the "processing noise" resulting from the expansion process. While the lack of statistical significance from the main effect of Duration might lead one to believe the "noise" had no negative influence (as well as no positive influence), it was possible, however, that the "noise" masked some real positive influence due to durational increase. It was recognized that in both tasks the vocabulary being sampled was more likely a lexical vocabulary, not a linguistic vocabulary. It could be argued that, for this reason, the tasks were assessing only the original voca— bulary development of the subjects and not his "available vocabulary." When the general familiarity or meaningfulness of the words within the first two levels of hearing vocabu- lary are examined, this argument seemed weak. The tasks, as presented, may have been more difficult than typical auditory language reception because of the absence of a linguistic context from which to draw additional meaningful cues. -109- The hearing vocabulary levels, as defined in this study, certainly did account for a highly significant amount of the variability in the performance of the word recog- nition task, as was anticipated on the basis of previous research.1 As indicated by the absence of statistical significance in the Duration x HVL interaction, the influ- ence of Duration was not sufficient to disturb the strong systematic influence of hearing vocabulary levels or the meaningfulness of the stimuli. In fact, the levels of hearing vocabulary, as construed in this experiment, may have been so pervasive as to mask the influence of the experimental treatment, especially in the second and third levels. Both of these levels encompassed an extremely broad range of meaningfulness, in terms of the frequency with which the words within the levels are used. Had levels been defined on more homogeneous groups of words (re: fre— quency of usage) perhaps the interaction of durations within hearing vocabulary levels would have been significant in more than just statistical sense. Nevertheless, at the durational increases employed, no significant positive influence on any reduction in avail- able vocabulary or retention span was observed. It was 1Joseph M. Wepman,I§£.§l., "Psycholinguistic Study of Aphasia: A Revision of the Concept of Anomia," JSHD, XXI (1956), pp. 468-77. Hildred Schuell, James Jenkins and Lydia Landis, "Relationship Between Auditory Compre- hension and Word Frequency in Aphasia," JSHR, IV (1961), pp. 30-36. -110- not surprising that durational increases did not affect all subjects,1 since the level of auditory language function was thought to be a variable affecting any possible posi- tive effect from duration increases. Scores on the Auditory Disturbance Section of the MTDDA did predict those subjects who would do well on the tasks of this study and those who would not. This was indicated in the significant main effects of Groups in both analyses of variance. Of course, test performance and task performance were not totally indepen- dent. The MTDDA section used includes tests that reportedly measure the same dimensions of the language deficit as the tasks of this experiment.2 However, pattern of performance within groups deserved close examination. Several very interesting observations were made within the group of subjects with high auditory language disturbance at the several durations. There seemed to be little question that their behavior on the tasks was dif— ferent from the behavior of the other group of subjects. The difference was not just in level of performance but in the kind of performance. This latter type of difference is eSpecially true of their performance on the word recognition task. It was lIbid. 2Hildred Schuell, James J. Jenkins, Edward Jimenez- Pabon, Aphasia in Adults: Diagnosis, Prggposis, and Treat- ment (New YOrk: Harper and Row Publishers, 1964), pp. 340-41. ~111- tfluis HALD Group that appeared to generate the statistically sjugnificant interactions that were obtained. It was diffi— CLilt to attach meaning to the interaction of groups within dharations. However, a visual inspection of performances of tflue HALD Group at each of the hearing vocabulary levels across 1ihe four durations revealed interesting patterns, even “though the magnitude of the variations were small. -At IEVL II, the positive change associated with the 125% dura- ‘tion was contrasted with the relatively equivalent mean :scores the group achieved at HVL I. For less meaningful vvords a slight increase in duration effected a slight im- Iprovement in performance, after which the group performance vvorsened at the higher levels of duration. In the plot of Dneans for the least meaningful words, HVL III, a slow posi- t:ive change was observed that reached its peak at 150% diuration. Again, for the least meaningful words, a still Egreater increase in duration was required to make the lin- Sguistic significance of the stimulus available to the sub— :ject with poor auditory language function. Perhaps these (abservations do not deserve to be called trends, but at ILeast they were patterns which attracted the attention of ‘the investigator. When the inter-task relationships were considered, .it was again this HALD Group that attracted first attention. .All of the significant correlations fell in this group. 'Ihis significant degree of association may reflect a -112- general low level of language function. The apparent lack of significant association between task performance in the other group of subjects, the LALD Group, provided some support for the notion that reduced available vocabulary and impaired retention span are two basic characteristics of the aphasic language deficit which tend to improve or oper— ate with some degree of independence as auditory language function improves. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS This study was concerned with an investigation of a clinical procedure used to facilitate the auditory language reception of aphasic adults. The influence of increased stimulus durations was assessed in relation to the meaningfulness of the stimulus and the level of audi- tory language disturbance manifested by the patient on tasks involving word recognition and retention span for words, with auditory stimuli being presented on both tasks. Summa y Recognizing the importance of auditory language reception problems in the total rehabilitative effort with the aphasic adult, Speech pathologists have sought to develop modifications of the auditory language stimulus that will make the stimulus more meaningful and more ade- quate for the aphasic adult. .One modification that has been recommended, and appeared to have rather widespread clin- ical application, was increasing the duration of the words Spoken to the aphasic adult by increasing the temporal value of the phonemes within the word. Clinical observa- tion has indicated that some aphasic adults profit from -113- -ll4- this procedure at certain points in the recovery process. The purpose of this investigation was to assess the effectiveness of such temporal expansions in relation to other variables known to influence the reception of auditory language by aphasic adults. Two language related tasks were selected and developed for the purpose of test- ing the independent influence of the experimental variable and its interaction with the selected control variables. Control was exerted over the range of auditory language disturbance represented in the sample of twenty- four aphasic adults by separating the subjects into two groups on the basis of their performance on the subtests of the MTDDA that are intended to assess auditory disturb- ances in language. One group of twelve subjects was then regarded as having a low degree of auditory language diS- turbance, while the other group of subjects was regarded as manifesting a high degree of auditory language distur- bance. The groups were similar in educational achievement and elapsed time Since onset of aphasia but did Show a statistically Significant difference in age, with the high disturbance group having the higher median age. A standardized picture vocabulary test (PPVT) pro- vided a means of controlling a second variable known to influence auditory language reception, in addition to pro— viding one of the two language tasks used in the study. The word stimuli within this test were divided in a manner -115- providing four lists of seventy—five words each that were regarded as equivalent in terms of the chronological de- ve10pment of early hearing vocabulary. Within each list, three levels of hearing vocabulary were defined arbitrarily and were regarded as reflecting decreasing levels of mean- ingfulness. Hence, the independent influence of the mean- ingfulness of the stimulus word was assessed in its inter- action with the two groups of subjects and with the experi- mental variable of increased stimulus duration on this word recognition task. AS a second task upon which to test the influence of the experimental variable, a retention Span task was develOped. As this task was structured, a series of dis- crete words were presented auditorily, and the subjects responded by pointing to pictures of objects named. The pictured objects were equated in terms of meaningfulness and grammatical class (nouns). The auditory stimuli varied in complexity as the stimuli varied in length from two to five words, with all complexity levels represented equally at all four levels of stimulus duration. The tape recorded auditory stimuli associated with both tasks were assessed to determine if their recorded durations approximated a duration that would be expected in connected Speech and to determine if they were intelli- gible to listeners. Satisfying these criteria, the tape recorded stimuli were submitted to an electromechanical -ll6— processing that increased their duration to specified nominal levels. The nominal levels of increased duration were 125%, 150%, and 175%. These temporally expanded re- corded words, plus the recorded words of normal duration (100%), provided four levels of the experimental variable—- stimulus duration. On a subject by subject basis, the stimuli for the word recognition task and the retention span task were systematically randomized across the four stimulus dura- tion conditions, and presented to the subjects under con- ditions resembling an average clinical environment. The dependent variable was the number of correct identifications made by the subjects on each of the two tasks. The data from the word recognition task were treated by a three factor analysis of variance in which the factors were (1) hearing vocabulary levels, (2) subject groups, and (3) stimulus duration. A two factor analysis of var- iance was used to evaluate the data from the retention Span task, the factors being (1) subject groups, and (2) stimulus duration. An inter-task performance comparison was made using a Spearman rank correlation coefficient. Conclusions Within the limitations of this study, the inves- tigator believes there are certain conclusions that can be drawn tentatively from the hypotheses tested in this -117- investigation and from observations made of other outcomes of this research. 1. In general, increasing the duration of spoken Single words does not improve their recognition or reten- tion by aphasic adults. 2. However, the possibility exists that aphasic adults with poor auditory language function might profit from increases in the duration of words in terms of being better able to recognize the meaning of the words. 3. Within groups of words having considerable variation in their meaningfulness (re: development of auditory receptive vocabulary or frequency of usage), in- creasing the duration of the words is not likely to cause any general improvement in the aphasic adult's ability to recognize the words. 4. Word recognition performance and retention Span performance appear to function with a greater degree of independence as the auditory language ability of the aphasic adult improves. These performances seem to reflect on the constructs of reduced available vocabulary and impaired verbal retention span, two disabilities reported to be primary characteristics of the aphasic language deficit. 5. The language deficit in aphasia manifests a very systematic reduction in word recognition performance (available vocabulary) as observations are made across groups of subjects that manifest different degrees of -118- auditory language disturbance. A major factor in the sys— tematic nature of the reduction is the meaningfulness of the stimulus. 6. In investigations such as the present study, it is imperative that the investigator quantify and con- trol for the expected variations in language deficit to be found within any sample of aphasic adults. Research that ignores such variations, especially variations in the language modalities being studied, will fall far Short of providing the better understandings of aphasic language problems that need to be attained. Suggestions for Future Research From the observed performance patterns of aphasic adults on the tasks of this study under conditions of increased stimulus duration, several lines of investiga- tion can be suggested. It appears that a study quite Similar to the pre- sent study could be meaningfully replicated with a group of aphasic adults manifesting moderate to severe reductions in auditory language function. Selecting more homogeneous groups of subjects within the lower range of auditory lan- guage function would provide a clearer analysis of the influence of increased stimulus duration on auditory lan— guage reception. In addition to employing more homogeneous samples -ll9- of aphasic subjects, any repetition of a study like this should narrow the range of meaningfulness within the arbi- trarily defined levels of meaningfulness. A more linguistically oriented approach employing increased stimulus durations or expanded Speech might be considered. It would be interesting to study the influence of temporally expanded Speech on the performance of aphasic adults in comprehending linguistic constructions of varied complexity. It would be reasonable, if it is established that SXpanded Speech does have a positive influence on some aphasic adults, to examine the influence of expanded Speech input on the Speech output of aphasic adults. The working hypothesis in an investigation of this type would be that improved Speech comprehension Should lead to improved Speech expression. Perhaps, in this area, a means could be devel— Oped to test the effectiveness of expanded Speech as a procedure for improving the self-monitoring of Speech output in aphasic adults. A word of caution is in order regarding the use of the expansion process employed in this study. It is strongly recommended that investigators check the actual level of expansion achieved by the processing. The nature of the process is going to produce variations in the levels of expansion actually achieved. It is important to know what this variance iS with reference to the nominal levels of expansion being sought. -120- It iS this investigator's opinion that this line of research is one that needs, and is deserving of, further investigation. At best, this study has taken but a glimpse of an important aspect of treatment for the aphasic adult. Additional experimental efforts are needed to discover the clinical Significance of changes in stimulus duration on the auditory reception of language by aphasic adults. BIBLIOGRAPHY -12l- BIBLIOGRAPHY Books Brown, Roger. Words and Things. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press, 1959. Dunn, Lloyd. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test: Expanded Manual. Minneapolis: American Guidance Service, Inc., 1966. Eisenson, Jon. Examining for Aphasia. Rev. Ed. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1954. Fairbanks, Grant. Experimental Phonetics: Selected Writ- ings. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1966. Gibran, Kahil. The Prpphet. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, Inc., 1964. Goldstein, Kurt. Language and Language Disturbances. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1948. Hays, William L. Statistics for Psychologists. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1964. Harlow, Harry F. and Woolsey, Clinton N., eds. Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior. Madison: Uni- versity of Wisconsin Press, 1958. Head, Henry. Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech, Vol. 1. New YOrk: Macmillan, 1926. Jackson, J. Hughlings. The Selected Writing_. 2 vols. reprinted New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1958. Johnson, Wendell, Darley, Frederic L. and Spriestersbach. Diagnostic Methods in Speech Pathology. New YOrk: Harper & Row, Publishers, 1963. Lindquist, E. F. Design and Analysis of Experiments in Psychology and Education, Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1953. Longerich, Mary C. and Bordeau, Jean. Aphasia Therapeutics. New York: Macmillan Co., 1954. -122- -123- Luria, Aleksandor Romanovich. Higher Cortical Functions in Man. trans. by Basil Haigh. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1966. Osgood, Charles E. and Miron, Murray S. Approaches to the Study of Aphasia. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1963. Penfield, Wilder and Roberts, Lamar. _§peech and Brain Mechanisms. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1959. Schuell, Hildred. Minnesota Test for Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia: Administrative Manual. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1965. . Differential Diagnosis of Aphasia with the Minne- sota Test. Minneapolis: university of Minnesota Press, 1965. Schuell, Hildred, Jenkins, James J. and Jimenez-Pabon, Edward. Aphasia in Adults: Diagnosis, PrognosisL and Treatment. New York: Harper & Row, Publishers, 1964. Siegel, Sidney. Nonparametric Statistics for Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1956. Thorndike, Edward L. and Lorge, I. The Teachers Word Book of 30,000 Words. New York: Bureau of Publications, Columbia University, 1944. Wechsler, David. The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins Co., 1958. Weisenberg, Theodore and McBride, Katherine E. Aphasia: A Clinical and Psychological Study. reprinted New YOrk: Hafner Publishing Company. Wepman, Joseph M. Recovery from Aphasia. New York: Ronald Press Company, 1951. Wepman, Joseph M. and Jones, Lyle V. The Language Modalities Test for Aphasia. Chicago: Education-Industry Service, 1961. Von Ziemssen, Hans, ed. (Cyclopedia of the Practice of Medicine. New YOrk: Frank Wood, 1887. Winer, B. J. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. New York: McGraw—Hill Book Company, 1962. -124— Articles and Periodicals Benton, Arthur L. and Joynt, Robert J. "Early Descriptions of Aphasia," AMA Archives of Neurology. III (1960), pp. 205-22. Deese, James. "Form Class and the Determinants of Associa- tion," Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, I (1962), pp. 70-84. Foulke, Emerson, Amster, Clarence H., Nolan, Carson Y. and Bixler, Ray H. "The Comprehension of Rapid Speech by the Blind," Journal of Exceptional Children, XXIX (1962), pp. 134-41. Freud, Esti D. "Recent Trends in Aphasia," American Journal of Psychiatry, CX (1953), pp. 186-93. Geschwind, Norman. "Disconnexion Syndromes in Animals and Man, Parts I and II," Brain, LXXXVIII (1965), pp. 237-93 and 585—644. Jenkins, James J. and Schuell, Hildred. "Changing Concepts of Aphasia," Perceptual and Motor Skills, XIII (1961). P. 270. "Further Work on the Language Deficit in Aphasia," Psycholpgical Review, LXXI (1964), pp. 87-93. Jones, Lyle V. and Wepman, Joseph M. "Dimensions of Lan- guage Performance in Aphasia," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, IV (1961), pp. 220-32. Kann, Jules. "A Translation of Broca's Original Article on the Location of the Speech Center," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XV (1950), pp. 16-20. Luterman, David M., Welsh, Oliver L. and Melrose, Jay. "Responses of Aged Males to Time-Altered Speech Stimuli," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, IX (1966), pp. 226-30. Orr, David B., Friedmann, Herbert L. and Williams, Jane C.C. "Trainability of Listening Comprehension of Speeded Discourse," Journal of Educational Psychology, LVI (1965). PP- 148-56. Research Needs in Speech Pathology and Audiology: A Special Report by The Committee on Research, American Speech and Hearing Association. Steer, Max D., Chrm. Washington, D.C.: American Speech and Hearing Association, 1959, pp. 40-43. -125- Schuell, Hildred. "Auditory Impairment in Aphasia: Sig- nificance and Retraining Techniques," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XVIII (1953), pp. 14-21. Schuell, Hildred and Jenkins, James J. "The Nature of the Language Deficit in Aphasia," Psychological Review, LXVI (1959). pp. 45-67. . "Reduction of Vocabulary in Aphasia," Brain, LXXXIV (1961), pp. 253-61. Schuell, Hildred, Carroll, Virginia and Stansell, Barbara. "Clinical Treatment of Aphasia," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XX (1955), pp. 45—53. Schuell, Hildred, Jenkins, James J. and Landis, Lydia. "Relationship between Auditory Comprehension and Word Frequency in Aphasia," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, IV (1961), pp. 30-36. Spiegel, Douglas K., Jones, Lyle V. and Wepman, Joseph M. "Test ReSponseS as Predictors of Free-Speech Char- acteristics in Aphasic Patients," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, VIII (1965), pp. 349-62. Street, Barbara Stansell. "Hearing Loss in Aphasia," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXII (1957), pp. 60—67. Tiffany, William R. and Bennett, Delmond. "Intelligibility of Slow Played Speech," Journal oijpeech and Hearigg Research, IV (1961), pp. 248-58. Wepman, Joseph M., Jones, Lyle V., Bock, R. Darrell and Van Pelt, Doris. "Studies in Aphasia: Background and Theoretical Formulations," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXV (1960), pp. 323—32. Wepman, Joseph M., Bock, R. Darrell, Jones, Lyle V., and Van Pelt, Doris. "Psycholinguistic Study of Aphasia: A Revision of the Concept of Anomia," Journal of Speech and HearinngisorderS, XXI (1956), pp. 468-77. Unpublished Materials Allen, Hugh 8.. Jr. "The Eltro Information Rate Changer Mark II: Simple Quality Speech Compression," Paper presented at the Conference on Compressed Speech, University of Louisville, October 19, 1966 (Mimeo- graphed). -126- Foulke, Emerson and Sticht, Thomas. "A Review of Research on Time Compressed Speech." From the Proceedings of the Louisville Conference on Time Compressed Speech, University of Louisville, 1967 (Mimeographed). Kurtzrock, George H. "The Effects of Time and Frequency Distortion upon Word Intelligibility." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Illinois, 1956. Michigan State University: Agricultural Experiment Station. "STAT Series Description No. 14: Analysis of Variance with Equal Frequency in Each Cell." E. Lansing, Michigan State University, 1968. Perry, Peter S. and Boswell, Nathalie 8. "Adult Aphasia Performance of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test." Paper presented at the American Speech and Hearing Association Convention, November 3, 1967. Perry, Peter S. Personal communication, November, 1967. APPENDICES -127- APPENDIX A FURTHER DESCRIPTION OF PROCESS BY WHICH STIMULUS DURATIONS WERE INCREASED This description will begin with a general discus- sion of the mechanical process, followed by a discussion of the electromechanical aspects of the process that account for the temporal expansions. In Figure 3 is a schematic of the device. FIGURE 3.--Schematic of Speech Expansion Device. . 0) 2 l 633;. j 33.: —’ 0 C(35 u dTfib msaCho a? r %8“§8 naasoch’ The tape is directed to the surface of the cylinder (A) in such a manner as to make contact with one-quarter of the cylinder. The tape is driven by the roller and capstan (B). The tape supply and take-up mechanism, an independent unit, are represented at (S) and (T). When the device is activated for some level of ex- pansion, the tape is advanced in the direction indicated. -128- -129- The cylinder rotates in a clockwise direction. The relative velocity of the tape transport and the effective linear velocity of the head rotation is maintained at 15 ips--the Speed at which the Signal on the tape had been recorded. AS the tape tranSport Speed is reduced for greater levels of expansion, the rotational Speed of the cylinder is in— creased. Hence the speed of the tape relative to the surface of the cylinder (and reproduce heads) is held constant at 15 ips. From the schematic diagram, it can be imagined that each of the heads, in turn, makes contact with the tape and that only one head is in contact with the tape at any moment. A Single control knob regulates the tape Speed and head Speed synchronously to maintain the relative Speed of 15 ips. The calibration of the control knob is in percentage of original time expansion or compression. The frequency response is reported to be essentially flat to 15000 Hz at all four reproduce heads. The increases in duration can be produced over a range extending up to approximately 200% of the duration of the original signal. If a word had an initial duration of 500 msec., expanded version of the word could be produced which would have durations ranging up to 1000 msec. In the sampling process associated with the production of the expansions, several aspects of the samples themselves need to be understood. First, the temporal value of the acoustic patterns within each sampled segment of the original Signal or word is increased as the percentage of desired expansion is increased. If a segment of the original recording had a duration in real time of 30 msec., and the Signal was being processed for 150% expansion, this segment would have a duration of 45 msec. in the expanded reproduction. Its temporal value now equals 150% of its temporal value in the original recording. The increase in the temporal value of the acoustic patterns in each segment is a function of the reduction in the tape transport velocity. It is necessary to make such reductions as greater expansion levels are sought. Where one inch of the recorded word represented 1/15 of a second at its recorded velocity of 15 ipS, one inch of the tape recorded Signal transported at a reproduc- tion velocity of 7.5 ips now represents 1/7.5 of a second, or just twice its temporal value in the‘original recording. This reduction in transport velocity during expansion pro- cessing would result in the frequency distortion and intel— ligibility loss (associated with "slow played" recordings of Speech) were it not for the compensatory effect of the rotating reproduce heads. This compensatory effect of the rotating reproduce heads restores the reproduction velocity to the level of the recording velocity. -130— This restoration comes from develOping a relative velocity between tape surface and the gap of each reproduce head which is equal to the 15 ips at which the word was originally recorded. Since the tape is being transported in a direction opposite to the linear direction of the head rotation, the relative velocity of the reproduction process (rV) is the sum of the transport velocity (VT) and the effective linear velocity of the rotating heads (VR), i.e. rV = VT + VR. In the example given, reducing transport velocity from 15 ips to 7.5 ips would require a compensatory velocity of head rotation equivalent to 7.5 ips. Achieving this relative velocity would give to the expanded reproduc- tion the frequency characteristics of the original Signal. It is necessary to observe, however, that as rotational velocity increases, the number of samples taken per unit of time increases. Each of the rotating heads is scanning a segment of the recorded tape in less time, hence more samples are taken per unit of time. This results in portions of the original Signal being sampled a second time as the samp- ling produced by a given head overlaps a portion of the tape already sampled. But, because the relative velocity with which this scanning occurs is equivalent to the velocity of the original recording, each segment of tape sampled is “treated" as though it were being transported across a sta- tionary reproduce head at a velocity of 15 ips, when in fact it is not. In the example being used, the segment being scanned represents 1/30 of a second of the original recording, Since the transport velocity has been reduced by 50%.from 15 ips to 7.5 ips. However, with the relative velocity of the reproduction process functioning at 15 ips, that seg- ment is given a temporal value of 1/15 of a second. In the expanded reproduction, the acoustical patterns in that seg- ment of the original recording now have a temporal value which is twice that of their original temporal value. The temporal value of the segment has been increased by 200%. Repeating this sampling process throughout the ori- ginal duration of a word would result in an increase in the duration of the word equivalent to 200% of its original dura- tion. It should be recognized that with this type of expan- sion processing it is highly unlikely that any two expansions of a recorded word will result in exactly the same expansion of the acoustical patterns of that word. This variability results from the differences that will arise in the defini- tion of segments from successive processing of the same utterance. The linear value of each segment is invariant and is determined by the distance (on the circumference of the cylinder containing the reproduce heads) between the reproduce heads. This linear distance on the Eltro Infor- mation Rate Changer - Mark II is 13. However, in successive expansions of the same recorded utterance, it is highly unlikely the beginning of that utterance will fall at —13l— exactly the same point in the Space between any two of the four reproduce heads. Hence, segments will be mapped on the word differently from one reproduction to t? next. The influence of such variations on perception are unknown at present. To illustrate expansion processing, the example used has been that of 200% expansion because it provides the clearest case. The assumption has been that the relation- ship between reductions in transport velocity and effective velocity of head rotation is relatively linear through the expansion capability of the Eltro device, the three expansion levels used in this investigation could be analyzed as the 200% expansion level has been analyzed. However, the rela- tionship is not exactly linear as indicated in Figure 4. Calculations based on the assumption of linearity from such calculations will not be precisely the expansion levels sought. From the experience in this study, it is necessary to refer to the obtained levels as nominal levels and Specify the observed variation from the stated value of the level which has been allowed. AS reported, in this study, a 5% variation from the stated nomial value was allowed. Without knowing the exact values for the transport velocity and the rotational velocity at the three expansion levels employed in this study, the calculations could not be meaningfully made. -l32- FIGURE 4.--EXpanSion Process: Tape TranSport Velocity vs. Percentage of Expansion.* Percentage Expansion 200% 17 5% \\ \ 150% \\ 1.25% \ o m C) m d o h u» N o m m o: H o :4 ox' F} or; u; F1 H H Tape Transport Velocity *from Allen, p. 9. "Eltro Rate Changer," amugum cmmsv won monm 3mm Adv cmupaflno a mmuu.monm Hmmn USmHmH Smupaflno monu mocom mso 3mm mmmam Ams moo G 3mm Hmflvaom GOHUHHQU cflmucsofi mop EmUHum cmo mmuu ummn own Ass own» w coupaflso sflmummo mcflu cmupaflno Hmflpaom o.H ”SOHumusa . B msasaflum MMMflM 1.. _ Amvs ocmamfl Isms mmn Ame moo laws cumummo Amas mocmu Ame Hams Ammo :mmss ISAC mags Ass Huum Ammv umHUHOm Aoav ummn fies mop Ammv nonommu Amv :mo Amv wwuu Ammo 3mm Ams Hams Ame mason Isms “mm: Amy cmucaflno Ame snap Aomv mmmHm Anv umon Amv 300 Ammo campuses 16v Hmmcfim Ass magma Ammo nm>flu Ame worm Ads umo .Ucdom mm3 mus» loam nofl£3 SH oumam B>mm mmumoflvcfl mflmwSDCwumm CH Honesz .mUHOB :50: on mcofluooamm mGHuHEHH pcm.mfl mo mmcflumu mmuoqlmxflosuona Um: mmEmc mm0£3 mmusuoflm haco mcflms mmumam om umuflm umme mnmasnmoo> muzuoflm wvonmmm map Eoum COCHmqu mmusuoflm om mcfl3oHHom mnu Eoum Ummoam>mp mmumam mDQDEHBm QMBdHUOmmd 02¢ mafifiqm mmZOmmmm ZOHBZWBMM m0 dem m NHQmem¢ Gmmgw w uwmc QSO Q HmU smmHOQSHHMqummg Cmo w HmO .mmHO£.Hm£0mmu.HHmfl -l34_ cmmsv w 3mm.mmmam.ummc Q90 w mmMHmsngmW~CHmUmmu magma w mmmam.um£ommu. msassflum mso mmHOS mmmHm HHmQ mmHOQ mGHH UCMHmH pmmc DMOQ GHMDQMO maflmu Hamn ummfl mogm mmmam umHUHOm umHUHOm moo ummc Huflm Unmamfl Honommu Cmo moo HMOQ COOSU mmmHm Hamn cmuoaflno mocmw moo GHmaGSOE mmmam mcflu Hmflvaom Hmmn M3. Smuflum Gmwsv Ass camucsofi Hmo Amy me03 Smufium Hamn fies Hmo Hmo Amy 3mm EWUHIV Hmmcflm Acs mmmam cmo Amv Honommp A.ucoov m XHQmem¢ -135— own» a ummcaw .Huam.mso.mucmm OHQMD o mmuu.3mm.mmu0£ cflmummo w 3oo.no>fln.mon GHmuGSOE w mmn.ummn manmu w mocwm.cmmsv cmucaflgu w magma msassflum mso GHmuGSOE mso mmuon Honommu own 3mm magma cflmucdoe maflmu manna mmn0£ Huwm mocmm mono wmuu mmuu 300 Sam umon Ho>HH cmmsv Hmmn 300 aflmucsoE won umon Hm>flu mocmm Hamn mso monm mcflu umon aflmucsofi ummCflm mumam amuuum mmnoz Amy Hmmcflm um>HH Amy manna Emufluw ocmamfl A¢v cflmummo cflmummo Amy Hmmn Ewufilm cmmsv Aév pcmamfl mow Amv amuwugnu A.ucoos m xHozmmm< mmuu a aflmummo.300.mocmm OOSm OW meonsaHmmGHN CHM#CSOE fi smmHogsmCHH -l36_ COOSU fi OOQ Hmmfl Q HO>HH monm w wmuon ~00C0W~UCMHWH~HMQQ OmHOQ 300 OmHOS QOQ NQMQ mOU OOHU mCHH Hm>HH umflpaom mocmm 300 cflmummo moo mmuu cmmsv Hwflomwu Hmmcflw magmu cmo women HOHUHOm cflmucsoa Samummo uMOQ 30m xflmn umo :mo mafia ummcflm Honommu mmmam panama wmuoz own man... HHOQ 14s mocmw OOSm Amy umOQ OOSm Adv OGHH cmmsv Amv OOQ HMOQ Ass umofl omoH "QOHHMHSQ 003m Am: HMOQ I “zoos m xHozwm me HHOQ w mUcmM~QHMUGSOE OOQm % BOO mmuoz w bcmamfl -l37- mafia w Unmamfl .mop.mocmm.cmm5v .300.Hamn.umon Hams w HHOQ.HHHm.GmU a Hmmcflm mmHOQ GHMDCSOE OOQm Hmmcflm umflpaom TUGGM @OU mmmam ummcflm damn Hamn HHmQ mmmam mGHH TOGO“ 300 Emu OHQMH Hmwfl Hufim pcmamfl 003m mmHO£ mafia mmuon UGmamH Gmmsv HHOQ HMOQ COODU HHMQ mso mmmam Huflm Cmo an. emUHlm :Hmummo Ass GHmuGDOE HO>HH Ams HHMQ emUHIN HO>HH Amy UMOQ mb.H "GOHHMHDQ GOHUHHQU Amv Hamn emgfllm 300 Ame HO>HH OUEmw Amv UMOQ -l38- magma O mmHOQ samQCsmMMHmscmwsv ~®O£m~mUcmM~mCHH OHQMU Q Hmmfl fi smGHHshgmggummC mmnm OW mGHH smog a wanmu mmmam QUGQM monu coupaflno Hugo NQMQ umoc mCHH ummc mow mmHO£ among ummc umnummu mso mozm GHmuQmu mow cflmucsofi manmu won mGHH HMOQ Hmmn wmnu umflommu UGmHmH mop Hm>HM a Hams :3 won Smuflum mCHH AHH Ev HHOQ EQUHl¢ OOQw :3 mmmam manmu Am: own A.ucoov m XHQZMQQ< -l39- Hm>umucH w>onm aflnufl3 mpHOB mamcflm Had .Avv.m¢o .mm.vav «muHEHH Hm>umucH m.hmm no sumo H.mmm an sumo: moHHm 00 *HMQQ m.>a¢ mm qu «.mmv mo Edna m.nmm no ammoo ¢.mmm mo pom v.mmm mo ummc ©.Ho¢ Hm mama N.mm¢ mo Mcsuu A.ommEv A.EEV COHHNHSQ SHUGUQ UHOS cousmmmz .mEmUH xmmu cowucoumu Eoum Umamfimm mvuoz mmDMOHUcH« mUHOB mdmcflm mo coflumuzo OZHQZOUWM deHOHmO ZH mQMOB m0 mZOHB¢MDQ mom mA<>mmBZH m0249mm00¢ mqumdfimm OB QmmD ¢B¢Q vm.voa u.Q.m ma.mm¢ "smmz m.mom em swan m.ovv om Hugo m.om¢ Ho umo ¢.©vm wv mmma n.mmm om com N.>nm om ammoo m.mmm mg G30Ho n.mam mo mmnmn m.o~m mm Hams H.05m mg ummc A.UmmEv A.EEV Goaumusn numcmq puoz UmuSmmoz UXOHCOU EOHM UQCHMUQO mCOflUMHSQ mouoz magmaawm mac 0 XHQmem¢ -140— .mEmuH xmmu coaucmumu Eoum Umamfimm mUHOB mmumoflchs Hm>umDSH m>onm canufl3 mpuo3 mamcflm HH4 .Amm.mon .o¢.omms "muflsfla Hm>umucH mn.oma ".o.m m¢.¢me cams v.Ho© wm sflmummo ¢.mmm on «Hmmcfim m.mom «m «mnmn k.m¢o mm um>muo h.sm~ 4m mammm m.¢ao ms Macmmmn >.mon om wsmcflums m.mmo om Mooomwm o.mmo hm Hmucsoo G.Hov Hm snag m.mmm om xomSmx «.mmv mo msnmm 6.46v mm manmm ~.mm¢ mo mcfluuflg m.mm¢ mo mcflamu o.mmm hm uSmEmmmo H.mm¢ mm umgofl3 N.Hmm on ucmvou m.¢mm mg mcflusom m.mmo om uouuos s.mam mo snag N.Hmm on magmas“ s.mmm om maucflnm A.ommEV A.EEV A.OmmEv A.EEV coaumnsn numcmq UHOB COHumusn fiumcmq who: Umusmmwz Umusmmmz mUHo3 mamsflm mo coflumusn uxmucoo Eoum pmcfimuno mcoflumusn meuos magmaasm 039 A.uGOUv U NHQZHAQ< Hm>umucfl m>onm cflnuw3 mUHOB wamcflm Had .Amv.hom .no.mmmv "muHEHH Hm>umucH HH.0h u.n.m mo.om© "cam: ¢.nmn OOH quEmHUGH h.mo> om mum>moxm o.~oo on mcflum>os m.oo> mm ucmpauom o.mmo hm mmcmEEH m.mmm om usmEm>mmqu h m.m>© mm coauumo m.mmo mm maHHMEQSm 4 ¢.vme mm msmommsmm m.mvm boa nouomfloum 4 m.mss mm Smflcoum m.oom ms magnumuflu o.ahn mm ooummcmx m.nmm Hm conuusuummu o.ann mm maflpasmmmm H.mmm me mcflaflmcoo m.oon mm mum>oamu m.mmm mm mmon>am m.mmh mm uwumficsmn n.mvo mm ooummsmx A.ommEv A.EEV A.oomEv A.EEV SOHpmHSQ numcmq UHOB :oHumHDO Qumcmq UHOB UmHSmmmz pwHSmmoz mouoz mamdflm mo cofiumusa uxmucoo Eoum pocflmuno mcofiumuso I mpuoz maanHSm mmune A.ucoos o xHozmmm¢ SOHDMHSG SH Hm>uwucfl may Umpmmoxm =coflumuuommcmuu: .Ime.mmm .om.memv "muusfla Hm>umch mm.om u.n.m Sm.moe "cams _ 2 M m.m¢m boa Gowumunommamuu _ 0.5mm wad coaumHSQEm m.mom mm mumuwuflano m.smn moa Suoumcwum e.mvo mm msoumuflcoo 0.5HHH oma coaumuuommsmuu ©.th mm Umumswmum A.oomEV A.EEV A.ommEv A.EEV coaumusn numcmq UHOZ COHumudQ numcmq UHOB pmHSmmmE UmHSmmmz mUHOB mamcflm mo coaumusa uxmucoo Eoum Umcflmuno mcoflumuso. (Ln mpuoz mannaamm usom A.uaoos o xHozmmma COHDMHSU ca Hm>umusfl map Umpomoxm =GOfipMGHESHHH= .Amh.amoa .nm.¢mhv "muHEHH Hm>umucH ha.m© ".Q.m ma.mmm "cmmz % H.oom OHH cofluommmflummmflo M 0.3m m3 coflmfimflumua _ «.05m «NH msoflumumamp 6.06m NNH umflmoHomamm o.mmoa mma GOHumGHESHHH m.©mm moa coflumgflfizaafl A.UmmEv A.EEV A.ommEV A.EEV COHumusa numcwq UHOB GOHDMHSQ numan UHOB pmusmmmz UmHSmmmz mUH03 mammflm mo cofiumusn uxwucoo Eoum Umcflmuno mcoflumusn mpuoz mHQmHaxm m>Hm A.uCOUv U XHQmemm APPENDIX D CALIBRATION CHECK OF EXPANDED TAPES The 1000 Hz tone at the beginning (I) and the end (F) of each tape (Al, A2, B1' and B2) had a measured length of 61 mm. in the original tape. The tone was of the same length on the Retention Span tape (RT), but the tone ap- peared only at the beginning of tape. Hence, the length of the 1000 Hz tone as reproduced in the expanded version of each tape provided an estimate of the extent to which the stimulus material on that tape had been expanded. The percentage of expansion was given by the formula Expanded mm. Expansion = x 100% 61 mm. The data below are the measured lengths of the 1000 Hz tone for each of the tapes at the nominal levels of expansion. Data obtained from read-out of power level recorder operating at paper Speed of 30 mm./sec. -144- —145- APPENDIX D (cont.) -__ fl: m E X P A N S I O N S 125% 150% 175% Tapes mm. expns. mm. expns. mm. expns. I 75 122.9% 90 147.5% 108 177.0% A1 F 75 122.9% 89 145.9% 108 177.0% I 75 122.9% 90 147.5% 108 177.0% A2 F 74 121.3% 89 145.9% 108 177.0% I 74 121.3% 90 147.5% 108 177.0% B1 F 74 121.3% 90 147.5% 108 177.0% 1 75 122.9% 90 147.5% 109 178.7% 82 F 75 122.9% 90 147.5% 109 178.7% RT 75 122.9% 90 147.5% 109 178.7% -146- Ho Ho Ho Ho so Mo Mo mo Mo HH mo HH sH Mo oH No HMNMNMHM NH No Ho No Ho sH NH MH MH oN HN Mo MH HN mo MH MH NMHMHMNM HH Mo No Ho Ho oH MH NN oH NH sN mo oN NN Mo MH MN HMNMNMHM oH No No Mo so mo HN sN MH MN NN MH sN sN MH MH sN HMHMNMNM M Ho so Mo Mo Mo NH sN Mo MH MH No MH HN mo MH MN NMHMNMHM M Ho No so Ho so MH sN mo HN HN Mo MH NN No MH oN HMNMNMHM M mo mo sH mo mo MH MN Mo HH NN mo HN MN oH oN MN NMHMNMHM M Mo Mo No Mo Mo NH MN No MH sN Mo HN NN No MH sN HMHMNMNM M so so No so Mo Mo MH Mo Mo HN Mo oH HN so oH HN NMNMHMHM s Mo Mo Mo so mo NH MN oH HN MN No oN MN so HN MN HMHMNMNM M oH sH NH Mo Mo MH MN oo oN MN sH sN MN mo MN sN HMNMNMHM N so No so so oH NH MN No Mo NN Ho MH HN Mo sH HN NMHMNMHM H HHMM HH so MH sH NN MN MN HN MN MN HN NN MN oN MN MN HMNMHMNM NH MN MN sH MH MH MN sN NH sN sN MH MN sN sH MN MN HMNMHMNM HH MH sH MN sN HH NN MN Mo NN MN Mo MH MN Mo HN MN NMHMHMNM oH MN MN MN MN mo NN MN oH NN MN NH NN MN NH MN MN NMNMHMHM M oH mo oH Mo MH MN MN MH sN MN MH sN MN NH sN MN HMNMHMNM M MN MN MN oH sH NN MN MH MN sN MH sN sN HH sN MN HMHMNMNM N MN HH MH sN Mo MH MN Mo MH sN Mo MH sN Mo NN MN HMNMHMNM M No MH HH sH No MH NN Mo MH NN Mo NH HN Mo HH sN NMHMHMNM M MN MN MH MH oH sN MN oH NN sN MH MN sN mo MN MN NMHMHMNM s sH MN sH MH No NN MN Mo HN MN oH MH MN mo oN sN NMHMNMHM M sH sH sH sH MH MN MN MH MN MN MH sN MN NN MN MN NMNMHMHM N MN MN MN sH Mo MN MN No MN MN Mo MN sN NH MN MN NMNMHMHM H HHMH MNH oMH MNH ooH HHH HH H HHH HH H HHH HH H HHH HH H MonHMMoH MM Ass MonHMMoH NMNH NoMH NMNH sooH BM MHMHH HMooM onHZMHMM M M M o o M z o H a H z o o o M M o M o z mMde ZOHBZMBmm QZ< ZOHBHZOOUmm ZO mfiumhmbm m0 mmmoom m NHQmemfi