£ww...§..n., .339 .m‘. .1 EM... 6?. :35 .1: flag“... .... . Mi 1.! I. IIL .19...er . ....,r..\.bh..;.l .v)... .(.|1L5... .xirp.§./rr...a. . .. 1. 9.... . >5; 5.133.}. , : . . w $.09... .5. 3.3.2.3 .55. . . . . . ....:_..zzm....,_..$a....>....u§?5 ,. . .. . "1:... , I. Q. . .. ..1.....: . .1 . . T. «Maw...— . . _. . . . i . . . 21‘s“..qu.ï¬.ï¬.un,mn.r Jumuwk axï¬m‘mï¬. Li? . 5. z... , .. E15.4.2232.:..>...v. LIBRARY Michigan Stan University . This is to certify that the thesis entitled UNIT ACTIVITY IN I‘HE SEPI‘AL NUCLEI DURING WATER DEPRIVAI‘ION. DRINKING. AND REHYDRATION presented by John G. Bridge has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degnPhl Psychologv W Major professor Date w 0-7639 ABSTRACT UNIT ACTIVITY IN THE SEPTAL NUCLEI DURING WATER DEPRIVATION. DRINKING. AND REHYDRATION By John G. Bridge To investigate changes in the firing rates of septal cells during and after drinking, single and multiple unit activity in the septal nuclei of unanesthetized and unre- strained rats was monitored with multiple electrodes. In rats adapted to a 23.5 hour water deprivation schedule, cells discharged nearly twice as fast before the 0.5 hour drink period as they did afterward. Electrical activity recorded for one hour in a control group of rats on ad libitum food and water did not change significantly. Sim- ilarly, in a group adapted to a 23 hour food deprivation schedule, electrical activity recorded during 23 hour deprivation, one hour of eating. and 15 minutes of food satiation showed no significant changes. Septal unit activity changed markedly during drinking if the animal had undergone 23.5 hours of water deprivation, but not if it had drunk water on an éQ.llD schedule: unit activity in rats deprived of food did not change significantly during eating. Sensory stimuli were relatively ineffective in alter- ing firing rates of septal neurons. except in the gustatory fflï¬Ã©g John G. Bridge (9‘53 mode with hypertonic saline as the stimulus. Units also appeared responsive to proprioceptive feedback from swallowing. The finding that septal cells are much more active during dehydration than rehydration supports the hypothesis generated by studies using anesthetized preparations: that one septal role in water regulation is to stimulate during dehydration supraoptic cells which then release more anti- diuretic hormone, causing the animal to conserve water. Changes in septal activity during drinking suggest that septal neurons influence lateral hypothalamic units not only as a consequence of the hydration conditions, but also during the drinking behavior per se. The nature of this influence is yet unknown. since septal units may either increase or decrease discharge rates during drinking. UNIT ACTIVITY IN THE SEPTAL NUCLEI DURING WATER DEPRIVATION. DRINKING, AND REHYDRATION By - .{N - ‘- I: John G .° Bridge A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1974 ACKN O WLEDGMEN TS I wish to thank my committee. Dr. Glenn I. Hatton (chairman), Dr. John I. Johnson, Dr. Lawrence I. O'Kelly, and Dr. Rudy A. Bernard. And special thanks to Keven Bridge for help in statistics. editing, and typing. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .iv LIST OF FIGURES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .v INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 METHOD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 RESULTS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .30 APPENDICES APPENDIX A HiStOlogy. O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O OLJ'O APPENDIX B Equipment and Suppliers. . . . . . . . . . .47 APPENDIX C Construction of a Multiple Electrode for Unit Recording from Unanesthetized and Unrestrained Animals. . . . . . . . . . . . 48 APPENDIX D Raw Data 0 o o o e o o o o o o o o o o o o .65 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 iii Table 1. LIST OF TABLES Page Effects of Sensory Stimulation on Firing Rates of Septal Cells . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 Confirmed Recording Sites 1 - 22 . . . . . . . 40 Raw Data 0 O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O 0 iv Figure LIST OF FIGURES Page Indicated are mean discharge rates per second expressed as a percentage of the mean discharge rate for all data points (within animals). On the abscissa is the recording time in minutes. Panel one (H20 DEP) shows 15 minutes recorded when rats were water-deprived for 23.5 hours. Panel two (DRINK) shows 30 minutes recorded when rats were allowed free access to water. Panel three (REH) shows a 15 minute rehydrated period recorded after the 0.5 hour drink period. Below the abscissa are the sample sizes of each group N 0 O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O 11 Panel A indicates the distribution of changes at the onset of drinking in firing rates of cells in rats deprived of water for 23.5 hours. Panel B shows the distribution of changes in firing rates at the onset of drinking in rats with food and water available ad lib. . . . . . . . . . . .13 Indicated are mean discharge rates of the chew— ing and swallowing components of eating by one rat. One swallow lasts approximately 700 ms. . .16 Indicated are mean discharge rates per second expressed as a percentage of the mean discharge rate for all data points (within animals). On the abscissa is the recording time in minutes. Panel one (FOOD DEP) shows a 15 minute period recorded when animals had been deprived of food for 23.0 hours. Panel two (EAT) shows a one hour period recorded when food was available ad lib. Panel three (SAT) shows a 15 minute post- eat period. N = 11 during non-consummatory be— havior: during eating. N = 6. . . . . . . . . . .18 Indicated are firing rates of one septal unit during different components of grooming.. . . . .21 Indicated are responses of septal units to a gustatory stimulus of hypertonic saline. admin- istered by substituting the saline bottle for V LIST OF FIGURES (Cont.) Figure 12. 13 Page the regular water bottle. Each data point shows the mean of 10 seconds. Open circles represent unstimulated non-consummatory be- havior. Closed circles show the rat's drinking of 1.2% saline solution. (Rat 20. tape 45). . . .26 Indicated are confirmed recording sites 1 - 6. . 41 Indicated are confirmed recording sites 7 - 18. .43 Indicated are confirmed recording sites 19 - 22. 45 Basic female Amphenol socket . . . . . . . . . . 51 A: Recording electrodes and ground electrode have been soldered to the beveled pins of the female socket. Bx Wires have been drawn to- gether through a PE-5O tube. straightened. and clipped to an appropriate length for recording. .53 Leads in recording position. . . . . . . . . . . 57 Top trace: unit activity recorded with 62.5me electrode. Bottom trace: the same activity as the t0p trace filtered through a 22 picofarad capacitor. Both sweep durations are 10 seconds. 63 vi INTRODUCTION Extensive evidence implicates the septal nuclei in consummatory behavior. Lesions of the septal area cause hyperdipsia in rats (Harvey and Hunt. 1965: Lubar. Schaefer. and Wells, 1969; Blass and Hanson. 1970). Electrical stimulation of the septal area reduces the water intake both of rats with water available ad libitum and of those on 23 hour water deprivation schedules (Wishart and Mogenson, 1970). Carbachol stimulation of the septal area increases drinking (Fisher and Coury. 1962). and atropine blockage of the medial septal nucleus reduces drinking (Grossman, 1964). According to Bridge and Hatton (1973). septal unit activity in rats anesthetized with urethane is usually faster during and after stimuli which are associated with or which induce dehydration (water deprivation: subcutaneous and carotid injections of hypertonic saline) than during and after stimuli which are associated with or which induce hydration (the 0.5 hour drink period of a 23.5 hour water deprivation schedule: carotid injections of hypotonic saline: stomach loads of tap water). The septal area also appears to be involved in eating and/or in controlling food intake. though not to the extent that it is involved in drinking. When the lateral septal area is stimulated with carbachol. rats eat more than before 1 stimulation. Food deprivation (or water deprivation) in- creases septal self-stimulation rates in cats and rats (Brady. Boren. Conrad, and Sidman, 1957). According to Johnson and Thatcher (1972), as food deprivation time in- creases. septal lesioned rats increase lever-pressing rates for food reward significantly faster than do unlesioned control rats. Although hyperdipsia has been reported as a consequence of septal lesions more often than has hyper— phagia. one study (Stoller, 1971) found that septal lesioned Holtzman rats eat more but do not drink more than sham lesioned or unlesioned controls. The septal nuclei also appear to influence various components of sexual behavior. MacLean and Ploog (1962) found that septal stimulation elicits penile erection; electrical self-stimulation of the human septal region produces a sensation of sexual gratification (Heath. 1963). Though researchers have studied several ways in which septal activity is related to consummatory behaviors, how- ever. the mass of septal stimulation or septal lesion data still does not allow us to describe the unit activity during these behaviors and. therefore. to construct and support logical hypotheses about the septal role in them. Soulairac, Tangapregassom, and Tangapregassom (1972) recorded with gross electrodes slow wave potentials in the septal area during dehydration. drinking, and rehydration, but their effort did not suggest how the septal nuclei function. Bridge and Hatton (1973) measured septal unit activity during different hydration states in rats anesthetized with urethane. But although their technique measured the vegetative aspects of water regulation, it could not evaluate septal activity during the behavioral act of drinking. Furthermore. Calaresu and Mogenson (1972) have found that the effects of septal stimulation on cardiovascular response can depend in part on the anesthetic used (chloralose versus urethane). thus casting doubt on the extent to which a urethane prep- aration reflects events in a normal, awake animal. And while Ranck (1973) has reported an extensive examination of unit activity in the hippocampus and the septal region. this experiment focuses on the hippocampus and on non—consummatory behaviors. When Yamaoka and Hagino (1974) measured the diurnal rhythms of septal unit activity in awake and un- restrained animals, they found that spontaneous activity decreases shortly after lights go off and increases shortly after lights are turned on: they did not report septal activity during eating or drinking. however. None of these experiments, then, adequately describes septal unit activity before. during, and after consummatory behaviors. There are two inconsistencies in the literature about the septal area and consummatory behavior. First, while studies showing that lesions often produce hyperdipsia (Harvey and Hunt. 1965), stimulation can reduce water or food intake (Wishart and Mogenson, 1970), and unit activity is greater during dehydration than during hydration (Bridge and Hatton. 1973) imply that the septum is hyperactive during deprivation and relatively inactive during satiation. other studies indicate the septal area is a potent site of self-stimulation (Stein and Ray, 1959). If electrical stim- ulation activates septal cells and if active septal cells are associated with deprivation, why then would animals eagerly self-stimulate? Another discrepancy is that while septal lesions produce hyperdipsia and electrical septal stimulation reduces water intake, carbachol stimulation of the septal area also results in hyperdipsia (Fisher and Coury, 1962). At present it is impossible to infer how the septal nuclei function during water regulation and other consummatory behaviors. That is. are septal cells relative- ly active during hydration. as the self-stimulation and carbachol stimulation studies suggest: or are the septal units relatively active during dehydration. as the electrical lesion and stimulation experiments indicate? A description of septal unit activity during naturally occurring consum- matory behavior may help us to understand these apparent discrepancies and related phenomena. such as the effects of stimuli and anesthetics on septal activity. To observe septal unit activity during consummatory behaviors requires a recording system capable of sensing cellular discharges in an awake and freely moving animal. Septal cells can fire at extremely slow rates, and to avoid a sampling error of missing slow cells, a multiple electrode assembly would be superior to a single electrode. In the present experiment, such an apparatus was developed and used to record septal unit activity before. during. and after consummatory behaviors (with particular attention to drinking). METHOD Thirty-two adult male Holtzman rats were each implant- ed with an assembly of six recording electrodes of either 62.5}lm diameter nickel-chromium conductor insulated with enamel except at the tip or 25,1m diameter platinum-iridium wire insulated. except at the tip. with teflon. One 125)um diameter nickel-chromium ground electrode insulated with enamel except for approximately 0.5 mm at the tip was low- ered to a position just above the septal area. See Appen- dix C for a detailed description of microelectrodes and their construction. Rats were housed singly in cylindrical plexiglas cages of approximately 35 cm diameter. The light- dark cycle in the colony room was 14 hours light: 10 hours dark, and to ensure that changes in electrical activity during a recording session could not be attributed to the rat's prior contact with a female rat. no females were housed in this colony room. Rats scheduled to be tested in water-deprived or food- deprived conditions were adapted to a 23.5 hour water deprivation schedule or a 23.0 hour food deprivation schedule for 10 days before the implantation surgery. Maintained on water or food deprivation. rats were allowed a minimum of 10 days of post-operative recovery before the first recording day. Rats scheduled for ad lib recording 6 i,, ,7 *__._—_LHQH___u———w_.—__—m . _~ .- had food available ad lib for at least 10 days before surgery and were allowed 10 days of post-operative recovery with food and water available continuously. A recording session began when the rats were two hours into the light portion of their light-dark cycle. Home cages served as recording chambers. and the procedure in— volved carrying the rat in his home cage to the recording room. removing the rat from his cage. plugging the leads from the recording apparatus into the implanted socket, and returning the rat to the cage. Electrical activity at each electrode tip was observed on a dual-beam oscilloscope. and the activity of electrodes with good signal-to-noise ratios were recorded on channel one and/or channel two of a stereo tape recorder. Most units were recorded bipolarly, using one of the remaining five electrodes as a reference electrode. Impulses from the brain that were sensed by the record- ing electrode went to an off-lesion-record junction box via three-strand phono wire. In the lesion position. the junction box could supply current to the electrode tip for a marking lesion: in the recording position, impulses from the electrode went to channel one and channel two. resPec- tively, of a stereo tape recorder via a low level pre- amplifier set to amplify times 1000. Impulses were monitor- ed with a loudspeaker. and an on-line counter was available to record the rates of impulses whose amplitude exceeded the threshold of the oscilloscope trigger. Recording sessions which focused on drinking and on the consequences of water intake lasted approximately one hour: 15 minutes of 23.5 hour water-deprived base rate activity were recorded. then the 30 minute drink period. and. finally. a 15 minute rehydration period. During the half hour drink period. approximately half the rats were allowed to eat: the food was removed from the cages of the remaining rats during this period. Recording sessions under ad 11p conditions lasted one hour. To observe the effects of food deprivation and eating on septal unit activity required 90 - 120 minutes of recording: 15 minutes of food- deprived base rate activity. a one hour eat period. and a 15 minute satiated period. Recording sessions began at 9:00 A.M. (two hours into the light period of the light-dark cycle). On some days additional activity was recorded: these data included up to 0.5 hours of exposure to a female rat, up to 0.5 hours of sensory stimulation, and (on the day the. animal was to be sacrificed) up to 0.5 hours of ether or urethane anesthetic. Sensory stimuli included tactile (touch. pinch. poke. or stroke). visual (house lights on or off). auditory (claps, shouts). gustatory (saline. sucrose. or quinine solutions: cold tap water). and olfactory (alcohol, ether. or litter from the tray of a female rat's cage) stimuli. Recording sites were marked with direct current lesions ranging from 8)¢A for 6 seconds to 30)JA for 15 seconds. Animals were perfused transcardially with 0.9% saline solution followed by a mixture of one part 37% formaldehyde and nine parts saline. To confirm the location of the marking lesion, brains were frozen and sectioned at 50lxm intervals and the cells stained with cresyl violet. The data from each recording session were considered those of a unique cell or a unique group of cells, even when an electrode in one particular rat was recorded from on two successive days and appeared to be monitoring the same cell on each day. That is. units recorded on different days from the same rat and the same electrode are considered related events. but not the same unit. Winer (1962) has suggested expressing repeated measures as a percentage of the total behavior or score. This method has an advantage that eXpressing activity as a percentage of a base rate does not: by assigning the value of 100% to base rates of each subject. the latter method loses the variability among them: the method Winer recommends preserves the variability among base rates. The data are described with means and standard errors and analyzed with repeated measure (treatment by subjects) design because this statistic is robust against deviations from the normal distribution by the population sampled (Welkowitz, Ewen. and Cohen, 1971, p. 141). RESULTS When unit activity was measured during dehydration, drinking. and rehydration. the cells characteristically fired at a faster than average rate during the dehydrated period (Figure 1). When the animals drank, the mean firing rates did not change significantly, but the variance of this mean increased markedly. That is, some cells fired much more rapidly and others slowed considerably, while the mean did not change significantly; approximately half the cells increased and half decreased firing rates during the initial drink period. The mean absolute change of discharge rates (without regard to direction) from the water-deprived base to the first prolonged drinking bout of the 0.5 hour drink period showed the largest change (45%) measured in any sequential comparison between non-consummatory and consum- matory behavior (see Figure 2). When the rat terminated its initial drinking bout. discharge rates returned during the non-consummatory period that followed to near deprived levels. Discharge rates during non-consummatory behavior then decreased until they reached approximately 57% of the water-deprived base rate. When the rats were allowed to eat during the drink period. the non-consummatory discharge rates only decreased to 67% of the water-deprived base rate. 10 11 Figure 1.--Indicated are mean discharge rates per second expressed as a percentage of the mean discharge rate for all data points (within animals). 0n the abscissa is the recording time in minutes. Panel one (H20 DEP) shows 15 minutes recorded when rats were water-deprived for 23.5 hours. Panel two (DRINK) shows 30 minutes recorded when rats were allowed free access to water. Panel three (REH) shows a 15 minute rehydrated period recorded after the 0.5 hour drink period. Below the abscissa are the sample sizes of each group N . 12 _ mmDoI mmzbzzz . om m¢ om m_ Co . on 02.53 - 00. 02:1on .. on. :mm szo nma om: BIVH ONIHId 'IVlOJ. X :10 "/0 13 Figure 2.--Panel A indicates the distribution of changes at the onset of drinking in firing rates of cells in rats deprived of water for 23.5 hours. Panel B shows the distribution of changes in firing rates at the onset of drinking in rats with food and water available ad_lib. m0 Luv. D A8 0 .00. o coco. coco. [GD 00 o 0 W0 0 Duo UF D .m 5P. 0 CF 3E o o 0 20 o o o o o co. coco 000 01: o o oo 0 R0 E2 TH E: _ k . r _ _ . A N D 0 0 0 0 O 0 0 O 5 0 5 5 0 3 | I - I . 025.23":— ozEDo 95?. @252â€... Z. w02<Io o\o FIGURE 2 15 Under all conditions. the mean changes in firing rates while rats were eating were much smaller than those during drinking. even when animals had been deprived of food. Dur- ing the eating period. it was possible to measure the dis- charge rates of components of the eating behavior: rooting about in the food pile. picking up and carrying food. chewing. and swallowing. Cells often fired faster during a specific component of this behavioral sequence, usually swallowing (see Figure 3). Cells also fired faster during swallowing in the ad 119 periods and after 23.0 hours of food deprivation. The lick-swallow sequence of drinking was too rapid to measure each component separately. For food-deprived rats. the apparent trend of septal unit activity during eating and during the post-eat period was not statistically significant (Figure 4). On some occasions rats were still eating at the end of the one hour eat period and appeared to be not yet satiated. Discharge rates of cells in these rats did not differ from the rates of those that had finished eating. And discharge rates during eating did not consistently differ from those during non-consummatory behavior. Food-deprived rats drank in short bouts which occurred at such different times through- out the recording session that representing them on single data points was impossible. Generally. the firing rates of these food-deprived animals changed slightly to moderately: the direction of the change was consistent within but not among animals. 16 Figure 3.--Indicated are mean discharge rates of the chew- ing and swallowing components of eating by one rat. One swallow lasts approximately 700 ms. m mmaol wozoomm 0m 0m 0? ON 17 oz_>>O.LI_<>>m n O 02.2,on u C CINOOBS 83d SBMIdS 18 Figure 4.--Indicated are mean discharge rates per second expressed as a percentage of the mean discharge rate for all data points (within animals). 0n the abscissa is the recording time in minutes. Panel one (FOOD DEP) shows a 15 minute period recorded when animals had been deprived of food for 23.0 hours. Panel two (EAT) shows a one hour period recorded when food was available ad 1gp. Panel three (SAT) shows a 15 minute post- eat period. N = 11 during non-consummatory be- havior: during eating. N = 6. 19 v mmDoI $52.2 om om on o . 1 o - on. \oz_._.<m/ +\ _ .. 00. $5 10m. 2m 2m goo... °/o ELVH SNIHH 'IVIOJ. X :10 20 During grooming. the discharge rates of cells usually increased or did not change: rarely did they decrease. Firing rates changed when the rat was licking its pelt or rubbing its head with its forepaws (see Figure 5). In a sample of the first two grooming behaviors in 10 recording sessions, 20% of the cells discharged faster than during non-consummatory behavior. 5% of the cells slowed. and 75% did not change. Firing rates of septal cells in rats with food and water available ad 115 did not change significantly over the course of the one hour recording session. Nor was there a trend within recording sessions: no mean discharge rate of the final 15 minutes had changed more than 12% from its firing rate during the first 15 minutes. With the onset of drinking. the firing rates of water-deprived animals changed more than those of water-replete animals. The mean absolute change (without regard for direction) was less under ad Ida conditions than when rats were adapted to a 23.5 hour water deprivation schedule: mean absolute change for water-replete animals = 17%; mean absolute change for water-deprived animals = 45%, p4<.001. To avoid overlooking cells which were not discharging during the condition at the start of the recording (such as water deprivation) but which might fire when the condition changed (to drinking. for example). background activity (multiple units with small signal-to-noise ratios) was recorded for one hour sessions in one of two ways: 1) when 21 Figure 5.--Indicated are firing rates of one septal unit during different components of grooming. 22 m meoC mozoomm om om oe ow - - - - C C O C C O U I0 I m>><n_ v.03 n O .Iwm .EDmZOU-ZOZ u 0 .5m.& v.03 u D mo<n_ mam u I cNooas 83d saunas 23 only one electrode was yielding good signal-to-noise ratios. two electrodes that received good background noise but no large units were recorded on the second channel and. 2) when no electrodes yielded good signal-to-noise ratios. data from two electrodes were recorded on both channels. Twenty-six hours of this sort of activity was recorded. One recording showed two cells that never discharged during consummatory behavior and rarely discharged during non-consummatory behavior. When they did discharge, it was usually during rapid neck movements. No activity was observed during the other 25 one-hour recordings. After six recording sessions (of three rats in two sessions each). a female Holtzman rat was placed in the cage of the male. and additional units were recorded from the male's septum. None of these encounters resulted in successful copulation or even in mounting or lordosis, nor did the animals mate on numerous other occasions. even without the recording leads attached. Because Holtzman rats are notoriously hyposexual, this line of investigation was discontinued after recording sessions in which four of the units increased in firing rates. one did not change. and one decreased while the female was in the cage. During the session in which the discharge rates decreased. the male rat sat immobile. without attempting to mount the female. In the other five sessions. the male repeatedly attempted to mount the female. After many of the drinking. eating, or ad lib recording 24 sessions, additional recordings were made during non-consum- matory behavior to observe septal unit activity in response to sensory stimuli. Because previous sensory stimulation of rats anesthetized with urethane (Bridge and Hatton. 1973) indicated that septal cells habituate rapidly. the modes,- intensities. and types of the stimuli in the present experiment were varied rather than repeated. Yet. in general. septal units did not respond to sensory stimuli (Table 1). Table 1.--Effects of Sensory Stimulation on Firing Rates of Septal Cells St ulus Responses to sensory stimuli were measured for 10 seconds and compared to activity during the one minute unstimulated period of non—consummatory behavior preceding the stimulus. A change was defined as an increase or decrease of 50% or more. To establish a control, 10 second 25 unstimulated periods were compared to the one minute of unstimulated unit activity that preceded them. Control data came from five measures of eight rats: stimuli data came from a varying number of measures of 14 rats. (Figure 6 shows the response of an individual septal unit to sensory stimulation.) Panel A of Table 1 lists the effects of all the sensory modes. Auditory stimuli consisted of claps or shouts: olfactory stimuli included 95% ethanol, ether. and litter from the cage of a female rat: house lights. on or off. were the visual stimuli; a tactile stimulus was a stroke or a poke with a pencil or finger. Panel B lists the gustatory stimuli in detail. Because the rats would lick aversive fluids for no more than a second voluntarily. their tongues were bathed with quinine and 5.0% NaCl while the animals were anesthetized with urethane (immediately before sacrificing). As the table indicates. the gustatory mode was the most effective stimulus in producing change: within the gustatory mode. sodium chloride appeared most effective. When rats were anesthetized just before they were to be sacrificed. the effects of ether or intraperitoneal urethane on septal unit discharge rates were observed. Unit activity was measured when the animal appeared anesthetized to a level appropriate for surgery (e.g., the animal did not respond to pinches or pokes). During ether anesthesia unit activity decreased to 58% of the non-consummatory activity rate prior to the anesthetic (p<:.02, N = 6, two-tailed 26 Figure 6.--Indicated are responses of septal units to a gustatory stimulus of hypertonic saline. admin- istered by substituting the saline bottle for the regular water bottle. Each data point shows the mean of 10 seconds. Open circles represent unstimulated non-consummatory be- havior. Closed circles show the rat's drinking of 1.2% saline solution. (Rat 20, tape 45) 27 m mmaol ON. mozoowm 0m O¢ o - d d mo_><Imm .EEDwZOonzoz n O .. 602 .\.-m._ oz_xz_mo u o o S d 0. mm 2.. S .0 ONE no 8 OMB O O N O¢G 28 t-test). During urethane anesthesia discharge rates dropped to 61% of pre-anesthetic levels (p<:.02. N = 6, two-tailed t-test). The effects of both drugs appeared related to the dosage. On three occasions unit activity was recorded for 90 minutes after the rat had been deprived of both food and water. In general. the effects of total deprivation were intermediate to those observed in rats adapted either to water or to food deprivation. For example. if cells at the electrode tip had increased firing rates during eating (after food deprivation) and decreased firing rates during drinking (after water deprivation). even after a totally deprived animal had eaten and drunk. the cells continued to fire at approximately the same rate as when the rat was totally deprived. In totally deprived animals. discharge rates during drinking did not change as greatly (from non-consum- matory discharge rates that preceded the drinking) as when the animals were deprived only of water. Electrode size (62.5;4m or 25)Am) did not produce observable differences. When more than one unit was record- ed on a particular electrode. two characteristics were observed: 1) Drinking occasionally affected the units differently (one might increase and the other decrease its discharge rate). but rehydration usually affected cells at a given electrode site in the same manner. 2) Units with larger action potentials discharge more slowly than those with smaller action potentials. This was true in over 90% 29 of the multiple unit recordings. and in each exception the electrical activity was judged to be recorded from fibers rather than cell bodies. Rise time and spike duration were used as criteria to determine whether an electrode was re- cording from an axon or from a cell body. (When recording from a cell body. the rise time is slower and the Spike duration longer than when recording from an axon: Chow and Lindsley. 1964). Only one type of response depended on locus within the septal area: some units that decreased discharge rates during rehydration would occasionally fire rapidly in "rushes" of 1 - 5 seconds at rates not reached even during dehydration. This type of response was observed only in the medial septal area. DISCUSSION The two strongest findings in this eXperiment were the changes in septal unit activity in water regulation and in drinking behavior. As water-deprived rats became rehydrated during the 0.5 hour drink period. septal unit activity decreased significantly. And during the initial drinking bout by water-deprived rats, individual septal units changed markedly: many increased and many decreased firing rates, although the mean remained relatively stable. The 23.5 hour water deprivation schedule was chosen for this experiment because it is a natural sequence of dehydration and rehydration, because many other researchers use this schedule. and because other studies have reported the details of its osmotic and volemic correlates. Hatton and Bennett (1970) have shown that as rats adapt to a 23.5 hour water deprivation schedule over a 10 day period: 1) the animals drink their fill progressively earlier in the half hour session until, by the 10th day. they drink in the first 15 minutes nearly all the water they will drink, and 2) the difference between pre-drink and post-drink osmotic pressure increases as a function of days of deprivation. The differ- ence between pre-drink and post-drink plasma protein did not change over days of adaptation. Hatton and Bennett also reported that by the time rats stopped drinking. their 30 31 osmotic pressure had drOpped to ad lip levels or below. After 6 minutes of access to water. rats are still hyper- osmotic: from 8 - 10 minutes, osmotic pressure is near ad lip levels. and by 13 minutes the osmotic pressure has drOpped below ad Ida levels. Rats in the present eXperiment that were adapted to 23.5 hour water deprivation and received no food during the drink period were under conditions similar to those analyzed by Hatton and Bennett. By the end of the drink period. discharge rates in this group had decreased significantly. though they drOpped more slowly than osmotic pressure decreased or volume increased in the Hatton and Bennett study. Unit activity in the group for which food was available during the drink period decreased less markedly (Figure 1), and firing rates of cells in the ad lip group did not change. These results indicate that, in general, septal cells are hyperactive during hyperosmotic (dehydrated) conditions and hypoactive during hypo-osmotic (rehydrated) stimuli: they corroborate an earlier report (Bridge and Hatton. 1973) of this effect in urethane-anesthetized rats. Firing rates changed considerably during the first drinking bout by rats adapted to 23.5 hours of water dep- rivation. These changes were smaller later on in the 0.5 hour drink period and when rats were on an ad lip. rather than a water-deprivation schedule. Furthermore, when rats are deprived of water for only 2 - 3 hours. and not adapted to this schedule, the changes observed during drinking are 32 also small (Ranck. J. B.. personal communication). Therefore, these results strongly suggest an interaction between drive state (water deprivation) and the effect drinking has on septal unit activity. That the septal units were relatively unresponsive to sensory stimuli except when the mode was gustatory and the particular stimulus hypertonic saline suggests that when their discharge rates change during drinking. units are responding to proprioceptive feedback of the behavior per se. It was not possible to detect whether firing in— creased during licking or swallowing. since both occurred very rapidly. During eating. septal cells responded most frequently during the swallowing component. If it is true that swallowing water also alters septal activity. then the greater frequency of swallowing during drinking than during eating may account for the greater changes in firing rates during drinking. The greater frequency of swallowing during drinking may also contribute to the consensus among researchers that the septal area is more involved in water regulation than in food ingestion. But while other sensory modes were relatively ineffect- ive in the present study. other literature shows that sensory stimulation can change septal activity. although it also suggests that septal cells habituate frequently to this sort of stimulation. Cross and Green (1959) have reported that many septal units respond for 2 - 5 seconds to tactile. visual. and auditory stimuli and frequently habituate to 33 them. Bridge and Hatton (1973) found that 10 second carotid infusions of hypertonic saline often activated septal units: these units frequently habituated after only one or two infusions. Brown and Remley (1971) found septal lesioned rats hyperreactive to thermal. sound. shock stimuli. but not to taste and light. According to Hayat and Feldman (1974). photic. contralateral sciatic. and acoustic stimuli alter discharge rates of most medial septal and diagonal band of Broca cells. Although their results report only responses up to 256 ms. they reported studying responses up to 5 seconds. In the present study all stimuli except auditory were presented for the duration of the 10 second response measure- ment. The changes in firing rates after saline stimulation of the tongue generally lasted longer than those Bridge and Hatton reported (1973). perhaps because rats in this study. which presented most sensory stimuli after rehydration. were hypo-osmotic when stimulated with saline. Since a rat's daily colony room life presents many visual, tactile. and auditory stimuli similar to those presented in this study. septal units may have habituated to these stimuli before the experiment. Often firing rates would increase initially after sensory stimulation. drOp below base rate for 2 - 3 seconds. and then return to pre-stimulus rates. as Figure 6 shows. This evidence. in effect. corroborates both the re- port of Hayat and Feldman (1974) that septal cells respond for very short durations to sensory stimulation and the 34 strong habituation effect first reported by Cross and Green (1959). Why do septal cells respond to hypertonic solutions in contact with the tongue. and how does this mechanism serve the rat in normal daily existence? As a recipient of information from exteroceptors. the septal area apparently inhibits responses to environmental stimuli. as numerous reports of the septal startle response or hyperreactivity phenomenon suggest (see Lubar and Numan, 1973). Septal lesioned rats drink more of palatable solutions than do control rats (isotonic saline: Donovick, Burright. and Lustbader. 1969: saccharine: Beatty and Schwartzbaum. 1967). They also drink less of aversive solutions (hyper- tonic saline solutions: Donovick, Burright. and Lustbader. 1969; quinine: Beatty and Schwartzbaum. 1967). Thus one septal function may be to moderate or inhibit responses to aversive or rewarding gustatory stimuli. 0n the other hand. taste buds also respond to chemicals in the blood (Bradley, 1973). By recording from the chorda tympani nerve, Bradley showed that when solutions were perfused into the tongue. units responded to sodium. but not to other solutions. such as sucrose and glucose. Furthermore. cells habituate to sodium stimulation when the solution is perfused into the tongue, but not when it is delivered to the surface of the tongue. Responses of septal cells often habituated to carotid infusions of hypertonic saline (Bridge and Hatton. 1973). yet in the present study. 35 cells habituated less frequently to hypertonic saline applied to the exterior of the tongue. When considered together. the eXperiments on the responses of septal cells to gustatory stimulation suggest that information about the salinity of the external and internal environments may reach the septal area via the chorda tympani nerve. Stimulation of the chorda tympani could activate the septal area. which would modify or in- hibit behavior by decreasing or increasing the intake of substances containing sodium. The response is mediated via the lateral hypothalamus (Miller and Mogenson. 1971; see below). Changes in the animal's behavior or reSponses could also elicit the charateristic response of unit activity to sensory stimuli: to increase immediately after stimulation and then decrease for 2 - 3 seconds before returning to base rate. Often when a rat changed behaviors. by beginning to groom. for instance. cells fired faster immediately after the change. decreased in rate below the previous mean. then returned to levels near the mean. Thus septal cells appear to fire in reSponse to a novel situation. whether it is a change in stimuli or a change in behavior. Septal lesioned rats are severely impaired in their ability to habituate to novel stimuli (Feighley and Hamilton. 1971). One role of the septum may be to transmit to the hippocampus information that a stimulus or a response has just been initiated. Hippocampal cells are able to respond to Specific novel 36 stimuli. for instance to novel water-related stimuli, but not to novel photic stimuli (Ranck. 1973). Ranck (1973) has also estimated that 75% of the lateral septal units are â€neck movement cells" which fire when the rat changes the position of his head. These cells did not fire when Ranck moved the head and neck of the rat. and the neurons did not react to specific objects as did hippo- campal cells. In the present experiment. responses similar to those of the neck movement cells were observed in units which were primarily in the lateral septum. Hayward and Smith (1963) found that septal stimulation decreases urine flow by. they suggested. activating supra- optic neurons and increasing the release of antidiuretic hormone. The rapid discharge rates of septal cells during dehydration and their slower discharge rates during re- hydration found in the present study strongly support this hypothesis. The septal area may also influence water regulation by two other routes. Septal stimulation can decrease blood pressure (Covian and Timo-Iaria. 1966; Calaresu and Mogenson. 1972). If the activity of the septal area generated by water deprivation causes hypotension of the renal artery. both sodium and water will be retained. Septal stimulation reduces corticosteroid levels in adrenal venous blood (Endroczi and Lissak, 1963), and septal lesions increase ACTH secretion (Bohus. 1961). In this system septal stimulation by dehydration would reduce ACTH, which would in turn lower aldosterone. thereby reducing the 37 retention of sodium and water. But the septal area is not involved only in water- saving: it is also involved in the behavior of drinking. Wishart and Mogenson (1970) found that septal stimulation can inhibit ongoing drinking; Miller and Mogenson (1971) reported that septal stimulation can alter discharge rates of lateral hypothalamic neurons. but the effect depends on the rate of the lateral hypothalamic unit. If the unit is in a fast phase. septal stimulation usually decreases its discharge rate and septal stimulation activates the firing of a lateral hypothalamic cell in a slow phase. That is. septal stimulation moderates the discharge rates of lateral hypothalamic neurons. This dual effect makes the septal function in drinking behavior difficult to understand. It is interesting that the septal area also moderates the intake of aversive solutions (see above. p. 34; Donovick, Burright. and Lustbader. 1969 : Beatty and Schwartzbaum. 1967) . Unfortunately. the bi-directional change of septal discharge rates during drinking complicates matters more. and no simple explanation is available. The present eXperiment indicates there are two distinct responses of septal units in water regulation. First. septal units fire significantly faster during water deprivation than during rehydration. Second. at the onset of drinking the absolute change in septal firing rates is greater when rats are water-deprived than when rats are on ad lib food and water or are food-deprived. Because the 38 septum has two distinct roles in water regulation. the apparently contradictory results of electrical stimulation and lesion experiments versus chemical stimulation and blockage studies are not necessarily incompatible. The septum's excitation of the supraoptic nucleus most parsimoniously explains why electrical septal stimulation reduces water intake while electrical septal lesions increase drinking. When activated. the supraoptic nucleus releases antidiuretic hormone. which causes the kidney to reabsorb water. In one role. then. the septum governs water retention. But changes of septal firing rates during drinking may also influence lateral hypothalamic cells and may be mediated by cholinergic synapses. In this role. the septum influences water ingestion. While this experiment has shown that urethane and ether reduce firing rates of septal cells. there is evidence that unit activity recorded in unanesthetized and unre- strained animals may be slower than in animals with no electrodes implanted. This evidence comes from the curious phenomenon that when two cells are recorded on the same electrode. the larger one (the unit with the largest voltage) almost always discharges more slowly than the smaller one. This phenomenon has also been observed in septal (Bridge and Hatton. 1973) or hypothalamic (Bennett. 0. T., personal communication: Walters. J., personal com- munication) units under urethane anesthetic. Apparently, the electrode is a heat sink, and cells in its vicinity 39 are subjected to mild hypothermal anesthesia. The cell with the larger action potential is usually closer to the electrode and will be cooled more. causing it to discharge more slowly. APPENDICES APPENDIX A Histology Table 2 lists confirmed recording sites (1 - 22). Columns two and three identify the 28 electrodes listed at these 22 sites by rat number and electrode number. The location of each site is shown on one of the six atlas sections on the three following pages (Figures 7. 8, and 9). The sections are redrawn from Konig and Klippel (1970). and the numbers next to each refer to the distance in microns anterior to ear bar zero these sections appear in the Konig and Klippel atlas. Table 2.--Confirmed Recording Sites 1 - 22. Electrode Site Rat Electrode 1 25 b 2 5 b.d.f 3 30 e 4 32 a 5 6 a 6 28 a 7 22 a.d 8 22 e 9 21 c 10 32 d 11 6 f 12 17 c 13 21 a.f 14 3 f 15 3 e 16 2 a 17 23 e 18 28 e.f 19 24 e 20 20 a.b 21 18 f 22 26 b 4O 41 Figure 7.--Indicated are confirmed recording sites 1 - 6. 43 7 Sl d g r r d nd . 4L: 45 Figure 9.--Indicated are confirmed recording sites 19 - 22. APPENDIX B Equipment and Suppliers Eguipmant and suppliers for microelectpode construction: 1. 5. Female Amphenol hexagonal 7-pin socket. $.86. and male Amphenol hexagonal 7-pin plug. $.97. Amphenol Corp.. Endicott. N.Y. Nickel-chromium enamel-insulated wire. 62.5;1m and 125;4m dia.. â€Nichrome" brand. Driver-Harris Corp.. Harrison. N.J. . Platinum-iridium teflon-insulated wire. 25)Jm dia.. 10 ft. for $41.00. Medwire Corp.. Mt. Vernon. N.Y. Three-strand phono wire. 30% shielding. 10 ft. for $1.69. Belding Corp.. Chicago. Ill. Liquid Tape. $1.09. CG Electronics. Rockford. Ill. Equipment and supplieps f0; bioelectpic data pecozding and analysis: 1. 2. Stereotaxic instrument. $750. Stoelting Co.. 424 N. Homan Ave.. Chicago. Ill. Preamplifier (122). $165: power supply (125). $335: dual beam oscillos00pe (502A). $13958 Tektronix. Inc.. P.O. Box 500. Beavertown. Oregon. Tape recorder (1028. $1196. Magnacord. Main Elec- tronics. 5558 S. Pennsylvania Ave.. Lansing. Mich. Electronic counter (5512A). $1050. Hewlett Packard. E. Hartford. Conn. Audiomonitor with speaker (AM8). $157. Grass Elec- tronics. Quincey. Mass. 47 APPENDIX C Construction of a Multiple Electrode for Unit Recording from Unanesthetized and Unrestrained Animals Abstzac; This appendix describes in detail the construction of a single and multiple unit recording system using either 62.5,am or 25,1m diameter wires as recording electrodes and a 125}4m diameter wire as an in-brain ground electrode. The wires are soldered to the pins of an Amphenol 7-pin or 9-pin socket. then insulated with Epoxylite or Insul-X. and the assembly affixed to the skull with dental acrylic and anchor screws: a 7 or 9-pin plug is inserted into the socket during recording sessions. and impulses reach the recording equipment via 3-cable phono wire. Comparison of recordings from the septal area using the two electrode sizes showed that electrodes of 62.5)Am diameter yielded a higher proportion of records with acceptable signal-to- noise ratios than did the 2511m diameter electrodes. while the 25;«m electrodes generally maintained acceptable records for a longer time. 48 49 Introductipn This electrode assembly has evolved from a number of previously described chronic unit recording systems. par- ticularly that of Johnson. Clemens. Terkel. Whitmoyer. and Sawyer (1972). who developed an in-brain ground wire prep- aration and have compared the performances of floating and rigid electrodes. and that of Chorover and Deluca (1972). who developed a technique for implanting wire so flexible that it essentially floats with the brain. Constructing the present multiple electrode is so simple that. after practice. fabricating a 7-electrode system requires only an average of 46 minutes: there is virtually no waste: and a novice can manufacture perfect electrodes from the start. The accompanying equipment (leads. etc.) is also inexpen- sive. reliable. and simple both to fabricate and to use. The animal. merely plugged into the recording apparatus and unplugged at the end of the recording session. experiences very little stress. Because the system uses either the 62.5 )um diameter nickel-chromium or the 25 )um diameter platinum-iridium wire with equal facility. their recording properties can be compared. Although either the 7 or 9-pin Amphenol sockets may be used. the 7-pin apparatus. along with the 62.51pm wire. is described here as an example. 50 Constzuction A female Amphenol socket (Figure 10) is chosen into which the male Amphenol plug (Amphenol Corp.. Endicott. N.Y.) to be used on the lead will easily fit. Usually the individual sockets are out of round when purchased. so that the fit is too tight for recording purposes. In this case. a metal probe is inserted into the individual sockets. and. by moving the probe back and forth. the sockets are rounded out until the plug fits easily into the socket. Then six 2 - 4 cm lengths of 62.51pm diameter. enamel-coated. nickel- chromium wire and one length of 125)1m diameter wire (Driver-Harris Corp.. Harrison. N.J.) are cut. Using a scalpel. 1 - 2 mm of the enamel insulation are scraped off one end of each wire. and the scraped ends of these wires are soldered to the beveled ends of the pins on the Amphenol socket (Figure 11. A): the 1251Pm ground is soldered to the center pin. The integrity of the assembly depends in part on three alterations of the Amphenol socket: (1) to form a single. rugged unit. the beveled pins. which are loosely held by the hexagonal plastic case. must be made rigid: (2) the pins and solder must be electrically insulated: and. (3) so that subdermal fluids will not flow into the plug-socket contact area and short the circuit. the junction of the individual sockets with the plastic which houses them must be made watertight. These transformations are all accomplished by 51 Figure 10.--Basic female Amphenol socket. 52 do._. > 00 ON 00 O on mo 04 2 3%.; me % 53 Figure 11.--A: Recording electrodes and ground electrode have been soldered to the beveled pins of the female socket. B: Wires have been drawn to- gether through a PE-50 tube. straightened. and clipped to an appropriate length for recording. 55 the following steps: a small portion of modeling clay is spread thinly about the base of each pin. and both the end surface of the plastic casing and the pins are covered with Epoxylite or Insul-X. Insul-X usually performs satisfac- torily. requires only a few layers. and needs no baking. but it sometimes bubbles and does not form as hard an insulating cover: although Epoxylite requires numerous layers (often 10 - 15) which must be baked individually. it forms a superior surface. To draw them together (Figure 11. B). the wires are inserted into a 1 mm length of PE-50 plastic tubing. which is drawn down to the base of the 125)Am wire at the center pin. At this point the ground wire is cut to a length which will allow the tip. when implanted. to reach a point just above the target nucleus. (The end of the PE-5O tube that is distal to the pin is flush with the interior sur- face of the skull after implantation.) Approximately 1 mm of enamel is scraped from the distal end of the 125 pm ground electrode. By holding the PE-50 tube near the base of the ground wire. it is possible to straighten the record- ing wires to a nearly parallel position. A small drop of Elmer's Glue-All is placed at the edge of the PE-50 tube distal to the pins (Figure 11. B: point c): the dr0p will be drawn into the tube and. after the glue is hard. further straightening of the wires is possible. With a pair of very sharp scissors. the recording wires are cut to 56 an appropriate length. depending on the depth of the target area. If 25/Mm wires (Medwire Corp.. Mt. Vernon. N.Y.) are used. they must be fused to the beveled pins with electro- conductive cement. Using a ring lamp with a 2X magnifying lens. it is a simple matter after 10 minutes practice to scrape with a scalpel approximately 1 mm of the teflon in- sulation from the end of each 25)am wire to increase the conductive area which will contact the electroconductive cement. Before implantation. the electrodes are collectively coated with melted dextrose: the dextrose hardens when cooled and allows the electrodes to be implanted as a rigid unit. yet after implantation the dextrose dissolves and disperses. See Chorover and Deluca (1972) for a complete description. The leads are made from four 30 - 35 cm lengths of 3-strand phono wire with 30% shielding (Beldon Corp.. Chicago. 111.). two Amphenol plugs. and one Amphenol socket combined as Figure 12 shows. The six conductive strands found in the pair of phono wires are soldered to the beveled prongs of plug pins A - F. the shields are soldered to the center prong. and the prong and solder are covered with Liquid Tape (CG Electronics. Rockford. Ill.). The shield wire goes to ground. and the six phono wires go to the recording equipment. To reduce noise that the two strands would create if they rubbed together. they are held apart with lengths of masking tape spaced 3 - 5 cm apart. If the 57 Figure 12.-— Leads in recording position. 58 TO RECORDING APPARATUS 9 Figure 12 TO IMPLANTED SOCKET 21.33::— v 59 counterweight is set slightly heavier than the combined weights of the plug and the wires. the animal draws the wires out of the way when he stands on his hind feet. and the only weight the rat supports is the 5.5 g implanted portion. Discussion The complete sysem is designed for simplicity and minimal eXpense. For example. this lead apparatus replaces the more expensive and cumbersome mercury commutator swivel. If a continuous record is required. then the leads used here should go to a mercury commutator. for if the rat turns in one direction five or six times more than in the opposite direction. the leads will have to be unwound by separating them at the plug-socket junction near the counterweight string and untwisting the wires. The wires usually tangle about twice an hour and require approximately 10 seconds to unravel. If a completely continuous record is not necessary. the present lead system is quite sufficient. Prefabricating the 62.5 pm electrodes requires less time and cost than constructing the 25,um.electrodes. But since the latter is a floating system. it retains good single and/or multiple unit activity for a more extended period of time than does the more rigid 62.51pm wire. Thus the 62.5}1m wires are advantageous for the sort of eXperi- ment in which a large number of electrodes are implanted for a relatively short period of time (such as that required 60 to map unit activity in an area of the brain during a specific behavior): 25)um electrodes are superior if fewer electrodes are to be implanted for a longer period of time (such as that required to examine unit activity during a complex learning sequence). To construct a 7-pin Amphenol unit with six 62.5 pm recording electrodes costs approx- imately $1.50. or $.25 per electrode: the 25pm system is $2.50. or about $.42 per electrode. As well as the purpose of recording. the cytoarch- itecture and discharge rates of the cells in the target nucleus may also influence the choice of an electrode. Chorover and Deluca (1972) have reported that all 51 of their 25)1m electrodes implanted in the olfactory bulbs yielded acceptable unit data. and Norgren (1970) found that 70% of the 62.5;um electrodes implanted in the hypo- thalamus gave good data. In the present eXperiment. electrodes were evaluated three to five times each. and approximately one of four 25lum electrodes and one of three 62.5)Am electrodes yielded good signal-to-noise ratios. Part of this discrepancy between these and other reported results may be caused by the extremely slow spontaneous base rates of septal cells: there are even "silent pockets" in the septal area. where there is little or no activity at all (DeFrance. J. F.. personal communication). In such areas it seems reasonable that the larger electrode would yield more data. The use of this multiple electrode system can 61 circumvent a bias toward selecting relatively fast-firing cells. a sampling error which can occur when an eXperimenter lowers a single electrode and monitors feedback simultane- ously to select a cell for recording. The electrode may bypass a cell that discharges only once per minute. for example. while the cell is inactive. This problem may be avoided by recording on one channel of a two-channel tape recorder from electrodes with good unit activity. while recording on the other channel from electrodes with neural background activity free from extraneous noise. but with no units. Re-playing channel two of the whole eXperiment can reveal extremely slow-firing cells or cells that fire under one eXperimental condition but not another. Septal cells firing as slowly as three times an hour (under urethane anesthetic) and 15 times per hour (in an unanesthetized rat) have been recorded in our laboratory. Because Norgren (1970) reported that unit activity during implantation did not correlate with that during subsequent recording sessions. units in this investigation were not monitored during implantation. However. a number of implants showed activity which was acceptable during the first recording session (on the 10th post-operative day). but which diminished in quality until. by about the fifth recording session. the data were unacceptable: this suggests that monitoring electrical activity during implantation may be worthwhile. at least if the target nucleus is the septal area and the electrodes are 62.51pm. This problem of 62 diminishing quality of unit activity may be attenuated by housing the rats in cylindrical Plexiglas cages. eliminating dark corners into which rats can poke their heads (thus bumping the electrode socket). Plexiglas can be shaped easily by heating it to approximately 1170 C and cooling it around a cylindrical object. Rounding out the individual sockets with a metal probe before constructing th electrode and implanting 25}1m rather than 62.51am.wires also prolonged collection of acceptable data by reducing the loss of good signal-to-noise ratios caused by repeated insertion and removal of the plug. Since most artifact noise in this system is considerably lower in frequency than is the signal. it can be reduced significantly more than the signal by filtering the current through a 22 picofarad high pass capacitor (see Figure 13). If the target area is relatively large. such as the rat septum or hypothalamus. or if the target is small and the eXperiment requires numerous control cells in the vicinity of the target nucleus. then this multiple electrode system is very satisfactory. But. especially when using the flexible 25}Am wires. this system is difficult to implant accurately. If a small area such as the supraOptic nucleus is the target. then a movable microelectrode such as that described by Teyler. Bland. and Schulte (1974) may be superior. 63 Figure 13.--Top trace: unit activity recorded with 62.5/1m electrode. Bottom trace: the same activity as the top trace filtered through a 22 picofarad capacitor. Both sweep durations are 10 seconds. ma 3&2 APPENDIX D Raw Data Table 3 (below) lists raw data from this experiment as entered on computer cards: the first card gives in- structions for interpreting data. Table 3.-4Raw Data 1 THFDE ADE F DATA FHTRIFQ PER CARP. EACH NTRY '" ' = 2 1-11. 14-25. 27-3q. Ao-sz. 53-65: 66-79. Ccm. 7&3833A83LÂ¥A§F nos. 3 an 1 Is ACTIVITY 10. R = HARE NON— CnmsUM.~ n = navnx. F =‘EAT. A . = GonnM. N = NOM- cor.uu. Col 2-s ADF TADE anTAGF. COL 6-9 AQF 5 “FAN qDIKEs pap sccown. COL 10 —1? ADF sTAMnARn Epnnes. COL l3 " 6 Is NO. OF 10 sec QINS WHICH couoosï¬ THE MEAN. DECIMALS HAVE ‘EFV 3 EZSS‘Q‘SHSBâ€:.LHSA‘JWFW5'9 “â€"“â€â€œâ€˜â€â€˜5“‘ â€Fm" †‘ - x : _. -A LACEs HAVE HE N DRODP u' 9 THE MEAN AND 3.5. DESPECTIVELY. E ED FRO paw DATA :3:§.:2...222.3:...229â€2 3 3 . :3. PO} R0]7018173416ROI491790279690170720030 ago 9 a 1 n02202SA71416002501850?93600990106721lsno30s:n333alawnasoaoa7svismolegzéwgzggggg NOSOSZZSOOOOZNOHRBZO332736N05682dl73166N090G2A00BPHEM067030Q04l§6N06°02513300305 N082531171856N0845226IPR46N3R7033003206N090033334366N09883l333786N105843R3483605 M116019502466 os ESTIsAanngélng? 31;?91n52q “3LT! EL = F 2 2 -n . 1 09a 090810168030a10271036003471013OSth04 712A3 a 239::9233352232:gamma:'3:3avzzézmï¬xszaozaznw‘9°"~°°“6°°""’*"“73"'~233 - - c . _. r ,0 _ :0 a o N114509810816N111006930676MIPAGOBH11903N175807838756N10 3 770676N111 0 quï¬aï¬g; £3850?2721§?ZA°39(173° nrD vuLTI EL = F 2 2 u. no , 0091HWOIA1lthnQRhHOPOWIRl1098690?3816§01076R Q n a 382?;{gzggzgzgggag}Zéggggfggzgg}gaggzgsnnwzglsg7o77snn17818:20A26n833si212347283 .: hNOl ?A 43†4° 2 ' N052024700001M05782330136300596221800020081618126302 n 5?0R5000?N"q0? 930000183 3.33%.:9...:2*33 3 2 . .. an 95046 OIPï¬ï¬o1E907000096H0200066701SAROZZBI2000 76R0 6 7 PnZRQIIROO3A6003PRA[R007780u3R0A481052h0017:Aab7096hnn391APA30£AAnuz§A2017gl?2%g 0094427310476Mc4hqonou7003nnao:G SOHDOOAOOHOOEkhOUï¬QHGHQ3l“7‘83099590'4r8?00000l10 N056009930216H655â€33ï¬00)0300C7P7dnnnhnlmannl7HOURTHF084Oln1303??fl06§?nï¬Â§0000?10 N06642402000?F0h750i))OIPHOOHQAA«banhï¬7NO7lOPRh7OOO3Gn7PR§7aOI?79007R§4??ROQ§A10 Agééï¬ï¬zmmï¬gv’miié‘fl232"3332‘;l§2‘:â€â€˜:“'mâ€1“WW:wwvrveomcoow0......†- ~ ' N ' . n s: unease? 46 . N107020§00536N108511000275N112075330876N0 ‘ ‘1 â€10‘0'“l704’5N107526R30446}8 655 66 2222227 1111111 169176 oqaooq 077007 01;:H00 0000031 0:. 00 (.41 415425 P337681 QDFMNE 2626:5616 10.03307 0000000 3070701. 33065603 0 7 0.569 0 07'6111 FHlXquqIO 7.470.963 .. Isa/1.701 â€7 00,000“ LRDEMCLNE .fih 3.56.622 9500700 14?...)100 99.1.1000 F.1I709700 11421796 0959997 E3705737 90126801 A 000 001-1.. TRDPEDDD III! 3311311233 111111111] 6711—517 onnrï¬qnn 00 00 En nrp .140 000 0098013701860n 01690760000180 O3Huvï¬3unoï¬lHfl 03 006900A58u55460 q 0 0 76216 C1 “69077 «.11. 0 97.00.66.034 00900090 7903.707 6. 60.971911: 317769401 100000000 4107QR§0 E35879 094 pOIPEgL902 AOnXYUOO‘i; TRRBDDDNN 6666666 1111111 666766 4R9037 014009 011011.. 399003 701094 140101 55.5.5.5.) OR nqqo 04 .H 574 6R014466. an032769 3m046§66 6M0600§ 359 766qrï¬44 Fnrxlrwqa/O 1087906: :1....14.ï¬..flfl11| 90)Onll LD.â€nHM.M.F.M. £6,666,666. R77qu1la 9190417 plill)†003.6).«9. 150.7..Hnï¬w-I T316564“ L666QIQ44 UROIORRO M D Q 350. 4 1L 094RPW1 00390.01. RDNJHVWE nu. 066,651.64 Fï¬dï¬rï¬77 40 n77R§69§ 21.07.11.111 00780030 7..5Q..H.))Hl.l. Harms/00)., SVJHQSQIA. 734.6025 qqnnlr.)7mfl SOP3579.U 0000000]. 9900“?†7666.66.66. R7480677 .597.P.7.7n 97407?R 60097577 14665544 373?...900 67269066 p0234790 AonnOOOI TRBDNNEN 77777777 11111111 Pogo—.5015 c.447qauls =I..1C.6u.0.l 09000.11 LRâ€NH..F.NN E6666366 3094367 0211565 4.15 4078 [equiv/493 7.17.2950?) LII-10900 â€9550900 â€177..“ 004 13:6)nl 0000011.. thMEHN 96666666 F.‘n7 77(10 â€ORPQ77S 1111021..“ 04277681 766673656 A 4 .14 09 7 01). 903108815 44101771 741310917 r57 9 R q 366 7077.0. 0.. 444 1111100000 00557377 646773768 P02468901 AOOOOOO‘II TRPDDGEEN 3888998888 RnnNGENNG 646666166 0079050rï¬â€”b 793442044 320240000 738707028 468463976 qaq47?_000 959374729 “QPC167730 1241367891 000000011 RRDNMEHVM 26665666626 “965323501 6690698304 31051000100 32082299.:25 PSI7NO NHL oQIBROORHa ' 676HO2534 606003849‘ hSSGUHOO 097M062 155607. 1 0 l 7 l 0 1 O 0 F2036802664 P02345789OI AOOOOOOOOII TBRDDNNNNNN 90999999999 lllll‘llllll 6646966666 Q46R733009 1907670094 1011617....400 9730179877 06RC7G1§01 3328739944 4.35.5.347100 “21.000091.“ OlIOHRwIï¬â€”H-I 7.145567890 0000000001 .1NGNHF.G 6163666616 10640747QQ. 10471067018 20 0013311000 5070897nw7n57 77770994 090 37035762325 43664377000 19110002300 70907654364 13345678902 00000000011 RRDGNGNGNGN S3 266133221666 06093000180 07064000674 33013000000 33077500777 2971.036 4 3757 07.17.76.991: 46 646645771000 FOQle/«I 01.45 an. 30 R76443232 =‘l7\1.urï¬.h.7nï¬q 07. 00009000011 LWD. GGDNDDNGN E663632rï¬3646 341500R7946 0.99740H04—HZU 61904079 00!! 7770.4qn77nn. 170.11.772.18 47 740127645567 L496 6 6 7.1.1000 U76H50700170 M977991331Q0 07.3.4c 1.- 770 Q 7. 0000000000.... BAUDHMHNMEN 0.66666R 6.6.66.6 F7.7..1691..r.5c..96 0796;5009697174 110017 7111000 080739., 0.5.16 372 704337Q C17 353'. H10 87.36.76. 766 34564341000 064 40a. (10010 5553 a 21102 4 502345677991 000000 .UrU.UHle RBUDNNHNMEN 76667.66 76.666. A1191. 07.7.3060. 1.. 994400900133 1200.17.92.10â€. 0:7?CIR 2C.ï¬..0.70 C7976 0117 11.8.1. 926.7 39.8041r74. 13456.4 4 421100 4q31IOOCIOOIhrH. F22322199861 702345667891 400000000001 TRPDGNNDNNEG 0000000000 7272227222 66166676.6 '2700003NOG 9134A1AFOA 102636Mofl 022R26N09 A20016M10 082946612 L) (A 1 7 l 7 38887186901 6 3 0 q .696007?6003 ',§2°hl7bBMUA' 26.63.66H5mh66 5472695536 78311393749 11192131110 20.5737H72C2 992611424 ‘01067HQ4 34qqq‘41‘l‘ F637790975 27.101674“ : 12.4..36730.‘ 0000 0001.1 LR.“ DVNGNNM 666.5..364665 _1:ASNI 2mm: HlSRhNO 'laaSun Alhflquï¬uo 19%08R600 0016h500 63HOQ6HO 8 n L) Sr) (0 4 .112 fï¬ï¬ï¬amwm A1754367 R6268469 498441170115 67156601413 70734578917 4000000001.... TBBDGNFNNNE .Phl RIB 7.66.6 666...).66 5.59 994488 0317 (1)4748“ Q7.0 1-0900 «1882132630 2635040231 Rll‘nï¬qpu/annun 7465—56935 F0q_ï¬qnunu.fx 00 7849246900 =124 â€H740 0|: 009 0000']. LRAHF..LLM.H...\H.I.N €6.66 ï¬hhï¬lï¬h 79 Q [40,6kyh 94.4 SHIHIHQ. 7390?“.00 1 WM R00h030187915Hnnuo' :Ahan, oqsï¬ï¬os7_ A36H07 01901 F00†OF" 05 3402355303406400750 T 6 66E039863800806E047.' 7 2 l ‘J 3 h 16F0545 4’3'1066 A O 7 97 0.6.42 637c.7r)6 1‘0 ‘10†on cl. 700 207.0 1.51.. WOOHQGIOQ? 73569G95694 0000000505 F2507 7.680333 70235679017. AOOOOOOOIII TRDFENNOENE 67 7722772 44 99999 99999 6666666666 272?8227 98988 9999 909 33133 4444 99999 7772772 77 ?2??7 ?72?7 7777277227 33333333 33333 3333 333 44444 4444 44444 666666 6 43366 63664 6411A66?9 68836666 6666 6726 66 6616 643 6?11 006796 3 80071 90930 760017708 136979?6 3747 6000 46 0308 346 3003 340986 2 40014 41087 230091401 97103939 3088 8008 09 4901 017 9007 1.1.7.000 1 90032 7.5109 000000000 00000000 1100 0000 71 1001 774 3031 877.789 7 00373 39049 7900119700 37303003 7177 0000 37 3700 797 1.000 498940 6 00119 9099? 810070919 3160R999 1161 0099 36 4109 676 3000 830060 0 91935 19073 339330311 78709161 4414 9193 64 4094 691 6116 333695 1 44111 72003 000000000 52317322 0000 0100 11 0110 111 ???2 949999 1 910?? 04798 ?QR946792 91R099n9 0900 9919 09 0009 040 9?59 439164 1 79335 44775 87QQI?Z66 74891441 0700 8980 07 4459 309 301 134680 3 74701 74801 134978401 13468902 3581 2468 38 2460 398 ?469 000001 0 00011 00011 000000011 00000011 0001 0001 00 0001 000 0000 FFFEFE D RNHFE RDMEF Rc.nDNFNGG RRHMENNN RDMN RDDN RN RDGN RDN RNNF 669664 6 64466 66666 67.761664? 66696636 6666 6666 64 6666 6164 6666 304141 0 09417 00901 009600490 07319702 4993 ?307 18 8811 ?016 A778 110676 3 77671 07843 301?0?490 441339?1 1?07 0?QR 00 1041 8000 9439 11?.001 5 43.971 77201.. 000000000 00000000 1111 1100 7.1 1011 107.1 7.7.51 370030 0 105503 108033 00n7n9939 07303073 3773 7707 230 0737 0000 3700 694360 0 0?7OR 90989 909011139 06393969 8113 6691 90 0131 9000 R199 R3708? 2 H7047? H787?Q 3?1191417 030C9811 3337 3077 H9? 7143 4870 09?? 437.994 4 C47711 C7720]. 000000000 41.33.7337. 0000 00 00 C11 0100 1711 7379 730990 9 26806 70347 965979650 39005080 0000 0507 00 0595 5658 0309 10177d 4 43013 83013 367780959 41719310 9999 9903 97 0185 6607 3143 13499.0 1 ?46 01 14801 11.496.64.00 134670.07. 2.570 74.61 75 «(49.5 2.9.90 7.4.94. 000001 0 00011 00011 000000011 00001011 0001 0001 00 0000 0001 0000 FFUFFH G HUHFF QDHFF HHFMDCNNN QQUHFMGN RNMM RDMN 90 QUNN RGHH QUMF 16676636 36 394?66 366?66 7166646636 766646666 36066 36666 366 36666 36336 36846 7008096 6 70075 00076 073734151 84717825 1099 9005 28 2613 6763 2096 0430616 5 35043 00046 012911171 3414438? 344? 0394 40 1046 0674 0?19 171.1010 9 6.7011 1.70.1.5 000000000 00000000 1101 1101 71 1110 3011 1.717 780.5770 7 00037 600.43 077988.300 00703700 71.1.0 3370 1.1) 0000 71.37. 3707.. 18569187 30 305581 319538 8227401076 359603100 36930 33be 338 35050 31336 33691 0947479 3 0910? 3496? 171411141 ARPIHGQI 94?4 3737 43 619? 3947 R?06 4334944 9 ?1?01 32311 000000000 44341??? 0000 0000 11 0100 1110 71?] F6709999 AQ A04974 A10396 A3300910?0 CRRUU99QO FHUOO 90090 A09 ROORH A9099 RRUOH 9707906 1 7139?. 9.93.... 7. 156970744 009Q7104 0000 037?. 09 639G 0191. 6048 =024HIOI :1 :14h91 :11741 =134967+01 =194$7QOI :?R70 21440 :24 :1187 :DRhO :139! 0000011 O 00001 00001 OOOHDOOII 00000011 00H1 0001 00 0000 0001 000) LFFDFENE L8 LHDUFE L8PDFF LDEFNFLGON LRQHNFHNN LHNNN LHDMN LPG LRDNN IRHFN LADMN 5.3663666 F.63 E66973 E66646 F.666266266 E66666666 E6666 .6666 E666. E66363 F6666 (.6673 9990177 60 19300 14100 714095096 3344??91 0347 3n76 497 HaAn7 9174 1101 8.97076...) 90 7.83.00 416.67 358011037. 4.3107773 12.49 096.3. 393 31876 48.11 410.... 9013000 90 71400 D31?10 00000n000 00010040 1100 [11? 71? 11000 111? ??44 4H319n? 71 F03003 E709PO F?540073Sn 03751703 E3370 F0173 330 31703 1010 7700 1376M466 116 L08 4533 â€blidQS L994238135 15.861.56.08 1.83.50 L0318 188:. 138693. 1302.9 11690 77....)79750 .175 625011 73.849 077.83.102.31 184264802 03.354 041.34 1231 .14 903.3 17.2.89 T1610 L53??999 L9? M31317 097110 M000040000 L44343?33 M0000 H0100 L111 100109 11100 I731? â€9139099 H38 176702 248710 1099300094 001705900 10000 10909 0095 HOOE)9 â€0956 H9069 “94.40791. “.00 (6.64070 HOCOD7 C0??1679?7_ M967797Q4 9.59.99 940.50 MG10 M7A94J. M6747 .4461]. 09197Q1 n9 11941 11700 134967401 fl?497401 1460 1&9fl 14? 11971 1460 1397 0000001 00 0.40"] 00)]... 00.7. d...........11 4.00....)091 On. On. H.001 4)l Air. .101 30...." 0900 DNcFHFFE RH QHHHF HWHFH DREFFFFLMN HHHHUFUN {HUN DHWM PM†HRGNH HDFH WTDN F?n63ï¬66 966 9666H6 667?3 F167166366 966636666 66666 96676 0666 D6nï¬hh 96616 96666 09.00.0436 F14 F3911n: 9.00006 008n070169. F771Q4Q07 947709 F.09U6 F866 €99,974 F9609 F6398 314.4794 0.4? 07413.7 10.91..th 07).“...4 .71 h??411114 11661.4 1?.409 DAD? 0.09394 0.9304 0003.9 03113000 71 70R44 691006 0000000000 00030040 L100?0 1700 111 10101 170) 911? 05373050 000 063.000 N30007 0070055700 0032313030 33.003 0006.0 0777. 030330 00700 Orv/.13 09986597 ?00 719H90 180906 0078627050 ?033h1535 033900 20900 7156 719410 23139 70181 F967?7?7 H79 H1739U 999346 F134117352 H96?08?3? A3376? H4903 H?30 H74l34 H?010 HAOPQ 4373595 60 91416 3???? 973070000 44343223 UOUJ3 0010 111 00100 1331 83)] 9000999. 1...) 73973 8.9?71 .58 97.99909 40.740997 UUOLU 3040. 500 030 50 9.53? 4990 3189840 76 14149 63.3.9.9 740044500 4.5010971 00.030 0.81.... 980 97007 074.... 6100 9.08....4491 603 613.570 603660 6074967801 7 .17. 4.57891 814697. 0039/ 00.51 003-371 014 6... 001.47 00000001 000 000001 000001 0000000011 100000001 IUUOOI. 20000 7.001 700001 7.0000 .COUCU QFFnFrE 80 9000†908%†HHHHFnï¬HH QRHDHFMU HHMHM QHDH RDH DRUM†8360 .QnWV T9669646 T66 T66266 T6£669 16621666763 T66AAF€66 TBA?66 T66?6 T566 TA4636 T6649 T41866 A79Q4QA6 A6? A92074 A03070 A91009070§7 AQ7n17343 A91076 A7309 A694 Ano?37 A3040 AR7K93 9??66362 946 991079 947955 97200111022 9154?5253 912085 99203 9048 943951 97800 996376 4110000 40 1303? 17110 0000000000 00000040 11000 0101 711 11111 ?1?0 31??? 69709.50 77 1.0071. 73.639 9900700971. 03777000 00077 0303 337 31.30.... 3700 03037 3998??4 66 139963 <14380v 12335647477 93661090 90916 40399 9396 R1101 Rhnn 01941. 77094877 408 9?7Q?R 977979 61?2917171? 710104708 933743 0339? 0981 379199 43031 904743 23423995 790 757772 772100 20000000000 344333332 300000 30000 3101 400100 41112 437312 9909099 9? 0n890 06491 0904073737 60995900 00000 9990 009 000?9 09?8 80900 F1767908 E24 E47578 F31744 F9190734693 [10917366 E95955 F4390 E491 E48764 59719 E79667 90134690 903 903470 903580 9023567890? 902356890 903581 90347 9039 902461 90368 902469 40000001 ‘00 400001 400001 40000000011 400000001 400001 90000 4000 500001 40000 400000 TNFFFEEF 790 TRDDNN TRDNNE TRRDGFNFDND T8900NENN TRNDNN TRRNN TBRN TRRONN TRDND TRRDNN 68 TAPF Ho QAT 21 H20 nrp quTI FL = c 3 2 800216100n696900496an307nhu037lH933nHRhH010H435007 7630132Hl3303°6R015833170H66H0 9019093930236q02l0ahshflh?6"n?Wthï¬nnhHï¬HOPï¬HAAHU)916Q099?hfl§003?68031R36670336§0 0039H603305H600411376I on363uu35AHHTn32AnnAHn400034lHnnan7anH00236MUHlHaQ33l016§n NOSPRH7R3n7Q600§n360ï¬00u06H0630439007R40061IHab7|173HOBQSH2670766Mo7[€42H00376qn N07298433n996no7414H830476Mn7HAH4670736Nn7676433nH16H043u99000776NURH6§16302RRSO N092052330736N097H anJOAhï¬Hl“306n33n426nl02Ra2670966Ml06092670396N10903900027650 N112266000606M113R6217O3H6HI13089170496 SO TAPE 91 PAT 2? FOOD nFD CIHGIF EL = n 3 3 POO3H026708963007HOP330096u0lIHOPH00766HOIAHOPI709863010004001106G023004HOOQ°691 n026n031707nAq029903671.3w6uuu1206332193wca6004l70096M04000a000266N0H7H07nnl73391 0066606000003306933 330676Mn73008000001N076H0H0000010091005670673N0°2803H0088691 M09BHO3R30936H103566171306nlnFH0433102bnlIRHO7UOOOOI 51 TAPE 5? RAT 22 F000 nFP FIN NCLF EL = n 3 3 N0090040014160011006500002N01403 317114 6N030804001136M032§06001066 52 TAPE S3 PAT 22 ann DFP VHITI EL = A 3 3 80030423347460006062n0497690390H~33292690[Poa983l94690l5839335706R02096367343693 GOPlï¬aonnnoanOPH)4300P1469620H1033263630320H3003Hg6flo37082173466F04HH37004076H3 F061053933346â€0h996â€lIPJP6uO7nOhï¬h7PAI3037PH47672755007606000000I€082§46933PR653 0098661502726M100961302944â€!02799000001I31086assooooznl06546002003N12266767485353 TAPE $4 RAT 22 ann nFD uHITI EL 3 3 N010049173166N01IHAA333Q66MOPP3393319R6N0P6041401699M030042503lRAN033066672033SA TAPE SH PAT 21 F000 nFo MHITI EL = A 3 3 8003024173636R007H2l6716QHH3!1H2380279H6H l6H2a0026363020018332236R026018672096HH 2028019172126803051967167600ï¬a24167 171 600 QROAQZQIl140101043672636N10292800178455 TAPE S6 PAT 21 ann nEp MULTT FL = A 3 3 M016237006929n0297316326360n212PHHoooo 2w023n3]832H26nn37923333206004n02417166656 N047S300035250052926000'02NOH402733223 buoHSOPHl730760056220000001N058H2690305656 TAPE 6! PAT 21 F000 nFD MHITI EL = F 3 3 ROO3H2OHOI5763007QPHHUPQahHOI091990SaleO140230019163017519R318H6001802100000361 8021013672336 024H2PHOIAH6HOPAOP35021l6DOHAQPA33DQS3DOQQOP733118600010250012a661 N101729000001H1028PHuODnO[Ml046263321460102023831766nlo3429674103Nl150388000026] TAPE 6? RAT 21 F000 OED MHLTI FL = F 3 3 N0010303332n3 OQan3lH3236600)6C3ISO3176M010029000002N012523203 89009283067121-62 005612QPHPSPOM057334‘30603 2H0‘3031332 '53(N0642P3001216N06832RA31 62 TAPE 66 RAT 23 HPO â€F0 MHITI FL = F 2 2 30022401839L69010742153F,OHHC]3Sao§§|6Q540[60439§?756Q0lR§34RV1936R020042G21§0664 30253343075780031H3H3HPHA6R.3119303u17hnnq3hanloH306nn37R39]00:46n0603379012366a 004733566053§nflï¬373°633HH3flnuAQW7dnl351Mâ€Â¢QHAISHPIOHHO5PO3Q3OIR76NOQQR3117176364 60557366000626056736300 08H‘975037000"WOHH2A3801953M060737022346M06PH3SaRIAQH6a N064736131036H36753H7UPI76m06053u7721’6N07]330670736N073H3l271126DO7HH33H3IH0364 ~076242100002007654IHnonnlM077A3PP6lH7HGn7Hn3Aa0[844N04003Pl22376N081033n3163664 N083929H01305Mo8H79163A nn6M.)°7H43P92Au6E09104613Pu96Fo°3042621616Eooa2a309105664 E0954300912F6~0968407HHQOAHI“003hannHAAGl0PP731SQuNl06234R8lnHHEIORHaono3nH664 6110040933026F11leIHï¬3whhull30343013QHNIIAH3 7312686116932302887Nl1833589293664 N120036461416M12l034u22006 6a TAPF 66 PAT 23 An L_IR MHLTI EL = E 2 2 30015172000768004219 21n76R007CPu07l23690102192312063013018780986901962122130666 R0182203H219630209216(2626R024619331236 90230)6h7l206qn32017P31Hl6n0337210H000266 N03S3IRPPO7AAH03~H17981nu6604201924|242M04a71R931336Mnu8317R£0APBNURIAIQA3IQ6666 N054816H611083056316333102MHH7OP£hHunthOFRPIQVHluS6F060017PSIPA6F0617IR7H000266 NO63H162206P6M066+I69UPAPSH1691197R]7Haw07t3llHï¬o1H66Mn73A16371006Nu75q15071a0565 N079018HQ.?006NOR?2[H320 360:,«4717230926u)37016231R3a603831IARRHIHNOQOPIRIHIPIH66 nn927237nnnan093hlu651a>6MO367lH60202660900l6490002ml004186§lthN103°lH6HlHP666 N10671970151361071237800n2ml)3717131606N1100 210500020111218300002Ml1231033OQH666 N1150162§21360117513'u000 UPN119019HrflnonlflllH 218000016119021200001N120H1763087666 N12301967l976 66 TAPE 67 QAT 2H H20 nFP HIHGLF Fl_ = P 3 80039617057qag0004.un 2A.36Hu|26.63397]6HOIHAAAH3HROAQQIRF 322349762021y3nnn300667 8023H3QH0616630263AO3%‘HHUPOHH3H3a7663031761672n35:101406617H2AHRU3HH601122H367 0037307672016PfluHJJH671+66dï¬'2HnuunoHH60066Hol330Hohnna67nPHOOUVHnuaIHOPHnonnPh7 006820600000?IJHHHQ:uunn)lHanxv7H3ï¬nnPGOHPooRHnnnoP3oHHonH331363F06000133042667 F06SonPPQn7F7uU7AUQPqu,c’0071H0P7Hlllama/20n2000002N07HH0nA7’316M07Hnn317007667 ~081001330406M032H0]l7)AG6HQHAH53nno63HDOH67o6onPn03H0H9)0A00P7IAM0800019H04RAB7 N0920007H0406F093H93 0))nnrounPUOHnn3a6Nl00HnPh71386mlOHHUPPHO63AN1IPH0333168667 N115303001246N118‘06671I63H1233nanPPH4MlPlIOPOOOOOPMIPBROPOOOHH6 6 TAPE 60 PAT 26 H20 HFD â€1““IF FL = u 3 3 R004067I7AHH60008036HO3176un|20H167AH2630|an36832696qn2ooaaa742hsnopanaal733966q nn2953ann3396an33333nu17LHHU3HH3517>.1hnn38016001uannn39617672126n04272 00216660 00991263333R3HUA639 an ï¬ï¬‚oPUJHIIRUSOOHOPNOHn7PH l73u9600H2023nonnnPM0H6H2433PPh66° 90Hfl63333PMAHH06*:129H 2436 66anwo) )£736un727>;H9 gpARHM0323363330MAM\)3HF3333427660 096007H3I 36F0°9033751304C‘3131100 Pnawlt‘mHP l PQGHNIO PP‘OOOO?M\ 009 00267669 N112118q0?3?hflll~“l3Ai33ï¬hfllIH‘PU>U3ROH [393133 69 % ! DAT 2 900590fl1712960’ 0023q0annun? n07110900nUOPP n10771090000? TADF 74 AQQWPO .R 7 7 noon H A m Plll q 07 77 P 1?]«AROPQQPQBIPIR6A0, H70 0‘9 OHACAUOQï¬QZITEPWWHUfla DAT ?8 77(98DOORPPAR37996H0 7‘70RH0qlgiaï¬oTA15uoau 119$6H105011001a46N107 anonDG069/27231u16MU7PT flFP A[WWW :a7600aa l ‘ . -wulhmuka 01â€45hï¬i77 OUOWOIHII7. O 3 7 3 HPO UFO 2» «79396n0087a71160163n q7935ï¬A030138H327Qï¬H0 nnnnnlmo50936301aaï¬nb 001405H115027502§flï¬Ml PflT 96Q7‘l q 4073 1 A0 L1H 2178H097§OBSOIAR6HIO_ PAT ?8 710000007C00021ZGQAquflnpq "ODOIFU&1006330§OHHOAQ 09560062916300002' 1n“; nï¬iwlsoenuaï¬ooflHclï¬uï¬uqyi ) 4' l qqpyq3nnogngqgsoo)unuvné?00026AaDO1261600! §000023097123334b9301060311726R6 3 = F EL i11r76R007q64674Q?6301756393RR§69017§5467 Q I’JGLE 5702RAVUJQOQP677OQ64132947330H01E017§a§81 ann DED PAT 30 3 3 EL = F §IHGLF 300299]60030029073677003513N040542R07R°6N04§93767897534 ann OED OED 1 012671116an 0 85 00200R491?11055485 066N122012§0317185 00.1 . 006R0260154307068§ Rh30714nus «u 91aa71a43u1 2 07 30 a0 30.1.01â€. Tandy“ ‘IQQQGDQ 11°6MU1 1173nOa 87 00445142516la00§750917306187 1210110 .587R202 6017R7R 2837811116 0011 LEQDHNNNE F.56286456 61.0).9 0 )6 9A â€/8352 llnlopnl l 1 0.000014 EHG MNHME 96668636 h. F1)“ P4139 1510 0Q)ï¬Â§:+ud7 Illa/Jail 012.1th .625 ?.4H...)u,é.?.n o) HO 3009766 ills/.1 11111 ROIHPAIDIQ [48379.9 H7. 2024A 7% WI; «50000 UO.U1 FHD M M.†MM T6 h’hï¬zh’hï¬zï¬ 371“? A a .1 z I AOOOOOOOI TRRnGNNNN ,5q15n04p92511anhqu onanUS7>11nnnnOPBO 00020087n0100000930 loAhnlnqq3n17nuuARQ “426N11R02h13186680 72a26R02annnnn1116R9 r o n 7 7 000 3*.6h'h) ‘UOJORUQ' 671. 706â€] TRDKZENN‘ 7O :11» 6.7 Q 1| 000001 LN EFF.DE 666.5666 70.: OAWSHOI lï¬ï¬ï¬ï¬‚UW 51175206 1250166P0 088001653 7aR2l18El 01.9449 5 91101117. gel/7R, 0 E343976 9025630 .AOOOHXUI TRRFDG F. ._ EL .RnRï¬bFU DOMAï¬EO‘O 01116806 lansnnw' 0786Nll SOQRhEU Fry-Fur.†N Dru-66.6.56...) CQ/u19.nun, 0'7 01 ?a A. lOlflanl T8.dR.r).d/0 Aculï¬ssc‘gï¬...) 94011:.)RI. 7117101 010fla700 69.1.1195? 1606:1909. £50.58 03 0 pozah‘eoz “00000.11 TFFEGFDN 7.69 “900 RON 666 36:. 607 IAUO 7:57 EL TBRQAAQDU' R‘OOFJHDU 39790u6mu An LIR ,,0900uR6H1 qqx 901 90! 902 RAT 32 inonanco n"00?M07 80696N10 ROQQ6902 LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Beatty, W. W. & Schwartzbaum. J. S. Enhanced reactivity to quinine and saccharine solutions following septal lesions in the rat. Psychonomic Science. 1967, 8 483-484 0 Blass, E. M. & Hanson. D. G. Primary hyperdipsia in the rat following septal lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970, 70, 7-93. Bohus, B. The effect of central nervous lesions on pituitary adrenocortical function in the rat. Actg Physiol. Acad. Sci. Hung., 1961. 20, 373-377. Bradley. R. M. ElectrOphysiological investigations of intravascular taste using perfused rat tongue. American Journal 9: Ph siolo , 1973, 224, 300-304. Brady, J. V., Boren, J. J.. Conrad. D.. & Sidman, M. The effect of food and water deprivation upon intra- cranial self-stimulation. Journal 9: Com arative gag Physiological Psychology. 1957, 50, 133-137. Bridge, J. G. & Hatton, G. I. Septal unit activity in re- sponse to alterations in blood volume and osmotic pressure. Physiology and thavior, 1973, 10, 769- 774. Brown, G. E. & Remley, N. R. The effects of septal and olfactory bulb lesions on stimulus reactivity. Physiology and Behavior, 1971, 6, 497-501. Calaresu. F. R. & Mogenson, G. J. Cardiovascular responses to electrical stimulation of the septum in the rat. American Journal 9: Physiology. 1972. 223, 777-782. Chorover. S. L. & Deluca, A. A sweet new multiple electrode for chronic single unit recording in moving animals. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 9, 671-67u. Chow, K. L. & Lindsley. D. F. Microelectrode study of residual neurones in the degenerated lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. Journal of Eggrg- , phygiology. 1969, 32, 116-126. 71 72 Covian. M. R. & Timo-Iaria, C. Decreased blood pressure due to brain septal stimulation: parameters of stimulation, bradycardia. baroreceptor reflex. Physiology gpg Behavior, 1966.1, 37- -43. Cross, B. A. & Green. J. D. Activity of single unit neurones in the hypothalamus: effects of osmotic and other stimuli. Journal pi Physiology. 1959, 148, 55h-569. Donovick, P. J., Burright. R. G., & Lustbader. S. Isotonic and hypertonic saline ingestion following septal lesions. Communications in gehavioral Biology, 1969, 4, 17-22. Endroczi. E. & Lissak, K. Effect of hypothalamic and brain stem structure stimulation on pituitary-adreno- cortical function. Acrg Physiol. Acad. Spi. Hung., 19600 179 39-550 Feighley, D. A. & Hamilton. L. W. Response to novel environ— ment following septal lesions or cholinergic blockade in rats. qurnal of Com a ative gpd Physiological Psychology,1971.7,u9g-504. Fisher, R. E. & Coury, J. N. Cholinergic tracing of a central neural circuit underlying the thirst drive. Scignce. 1962, 138, 691- 693 Grossman, S. P. Effects of chemical stimulation of the septal area on motivation. Journal of Com a ative and Physiological Psychology. ,195-200. Harvey. J. A. & Hunt, H. F. Effect of septal lesions on thirst in the rat as indicated by water consumption and Operant responding for thirst reward. Journal 9: Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1965, 9-5 . Hatton. G. I. & Bennett, C. T. Satiation of thirst and termination of drinking: roles of plasma osmolality and absorption. Physiology and Behavior. 1970. 5, 479-487- Hayat, A. & Feldman, S. Effects of sensory stimuli on single cell activity in the se tum of the cat. Experimental Neurology.1974, 3, 298- -313. Hayward. J. R. & Smith, W. K. Influence of limbic system on neurohypophysis. Archives pi Neurology. 1963, 9' 171-1770 73 Heath, R. G. Electrical self-stimulation of the brain in man. American Journal persychiatry. 1963, 120. 573-579. Johnson, D. A. & Thatcher. K. Differential effects of food deprivation on the fixed ratio behavior of normal rats and rats with septal lesions. Psychonomic Scigncg, 1972, 26, #5-#6. Johnson, J. H., Clemens. J. A.. Terkel, J.. Whitmoyer. D. I., & Sawyer, C. H. Technique for recording multiple- unit activity from the brain of the freely-moving rat. Neuroendocrinology. 1972, 9, 90-99. Konig, J. F. R. & Klippel, R. A. Thgwggr Braip: A Stereotaxic Atlas 9: 3h; Forebrain gag Lowgr Parts 9; the Brain Stem. Huntington. New York: Robert E. Krieger Publishing Co.. Inc.. 1970. Lubar, J. F. & Numan, R. Behavioral and physiological studies of septal function and related medial cortical structures. Journal 91 Behavioral Biology. 1973, 8, 1-25. Lubar, J. F.. Schaefer, C. F.. & Wells. D. G. The role of the septal area in the regulation of water intake and associated motivational behavior. Annals 9; gap Egg York Academy Q: Sciences. 1969, 15?. 875- 93. MacLean. P. D. & Ploog, D. w. Cerebral representation of penile erection. Journal pf Neurophysiology, 1962: 250 29‘550 Miller. J. J. & Mogenson. G. J. Effect of septal stim- ulation on lateral hypothalamic unit activity in the rat. Brain Resgarch, 1971, 32, 125-1#2. Norgren. R. Gustatory responses in the hypothalamus. Blain ReggaICh, 1970, 21, 63-770 Ranck, J. B., Jr. Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. Part I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Experimental Neurolo , 1973. 41, 461'5310 Stein. L. & Ray, 0. S. Self-regulation of brain-stimulating current intensity in the rat. Scie ce, 1959. 130, 570-571. 7L. Stoller. W. L. Effects of septal and amygdaloid lesions on discrimination. eating, and drinking. Physiology and Behavio . 1971, 8, 823-828. Soulairac, A..’Tangapregassom, M. J., & Tangapregassom, A. M. Anal se electroencephalographi ue sous-corticale des etats de soif et de satiet chez le rat. Q. 3. Acad. Spi. P is, 1972. T275, Serie D. 97-99. Teyler. T. J., Bland. B. H., & Schulte. R. A chronic micro-electrode and stimulating system. Ppysiology and Behavio . 1974, 12, 503-508. Welkowitz, J., Ewen. R. B., & Cohen, J. Introductory Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Academic Press. 1971. Winer, B. J. Statistical Princi les ip_Experimental Design. New York: McGraw Hill. 1952. Wishart, T. B. & Mogenson, G. J. Reduction of water intake by electrical stimulation of the septal region of the rat brain. Physiglpgy gpd Behavior. 1970, 5, Yamaoka, S. & Hagino, N. Spontaneous septal neuron activity in the rat. Brain Rgsearch, 1974. 67. 147-152. WIT! 082 0058 1 293 03 u" V.“ n†S“ R†H Nâ€! l I H H H I." H H