GENERALIZATION OF THE PAIRED-ASSOCIATE MODEL TO DEFINITION LEARNING Thesis for 951:2 Degas of Ph. D. MICHIGAN ETATE UNIVERSITY SaIaI'a AbduI-Menéen': Hafar I966 TH ESIS This is to certifg that the thesis entitled GENERALIZATION OF THE PAIRED-ASSOCIATE MODEL TO DEFINITION LEARNING presented by Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D degree in Education (mew/Mam Major (tofessor Date and H, me I 0-169 ABSTRACT GENERALIZATION OF THE PAIRED-ASSOCIATE MODEL TO DEFINITION LEARNING by Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar The Problem Verbal learning psychologists are primarily concerned with study- ing the basic mechanisms of learning by using nonsense syllables in highly controlled experimental procedures with adult subjects. The variable which has received the greatest attention has been the mean- ingfulness (m) of the verbal material. There is a reasonable consis- tency between the empirical findings and paired-associate theories, namely that m of the reSponse member of a paired-associate has a greater effect upon learning than m of the stimulus member. Most paired-asso- ciate theories predict the following order of the four basic types of lists H-H. L-H, HAL. and L-L arranged according to their ease of learn. ing (H indicates high meaningfulness, and.L low meaningfulness). Howe ever, paired-associate studies have yielded contradictory results con- cerning the effect of familiarization on the response members prior to the actual learning task. The satiation theory, the most promising one. predicts the following order HAL, L-L, H-H. and.L-H if the subjects are familiarized.with the reSponses. Educators, on the other hand, are concerned as to whether the material, the method, and even the findings have any objective applica- tions to the classroom. Accordingly, the present study stands between Salah Abdul-Mbniem Hotar these two extremes and attempts to test the appropriateness of extend- ing the paired-associate model to definition learning. Thus, the major purpose of this study is to test the hypothesis that paired-associate learning can be used as a model for learning arithmetical definitions. The acceptance of this hypothesis requires two conditions. First, there must be a one-to-one correspondence between the elements of a paired associate and a definition. Second, certain relations must be preserved, namely that the order of the types of definitions must be the same as the order of the types of paired-associates under different familiariza- tion treatments. The Methodology Feur arithmetic textbook series were used to gather arithemtical definitions which appear in grades five, six, seven and eight. Defini- tions with stimuli composed of more than one word or with symbols in the reaponse were excluded. Ninety-seven arithmetical definitions were finally used. Forty-eight of these definitions were numerical, and the other forty-nine were geometrical. The two kinds of definitions, numerical and geometrical, were randomly arranged. The stimuli and the responses were separated from each other. The m of the definitions, their stimuli, and their reSponses were determined separately by the use of three rating scales each of which was judged by approximately thirty sixth grade pupils whose median age was 1h5 months. Meaningfulness was defined operationally in terms of the students' judged familiarity and - ease of learning. The reliability coefficients were significantly dif- ferent from zero (Pa<:.01). The results of this step has been.used first to study the interrelationships between m of definitions, m.of Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar the stimuli, m of the reSponses, number of letters in the stimulus and number of words in the response; second, to select the four types of definitions H-H, L-H, HAL, and.LéL. Four sets of definitions were chosen. Each set contained four definitions, two numerical and two geometrical. One definition of each kind was longer in length of the response than the other. However, each list had approximately the same total number of words. The verbal familiarization material was composed of three sentences for each selected definition. In case of picture familiarization, the numerical definitions were explained by presenting three number operations, while the geometrical definitions were explained by drawing three consecutive pictures. The subjects who did not receive familiarization were desig- nated as a control group. Thus the four types of definitions and the three kinds of familiarization yielded twelve treatments. The subjects were 434 volunteer students enrolled in the seventh grade. Their median age was 151 months. The learning task was admin- istered by the use of a group procedure in which definitions as well as the familiarization material were presented by an overhead projector. Rate of presentation of the definition stimulus and reSponse was 5 and 15 seconds respectively in paired-associate like procedure. The course of experimental session was as follows: Pre-test, presentation of familiarization material for all groups except the control group, a learning trial followed by a test trial; two learning trials followed by the second test trial; two learning trials followed by the third test trial; two learning trials followed by the fourth test trial; two learning trials followed by the fifth test trial; post—test. During Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar the pre- and post-tests the subjects were instructed to express the meaning of the stimulus using their own words, during the test trials, they were aSked to write the exact words of the response as shown to them in the learning trials. The subjects answers in the test trials were classified into eight different categories with the assumption that these categories represented a continuum which began with a "no answer” and ended with "recalling the exact reSponse" and covered the different levels of the answers. 0n the other hand, the pre- and post- test answers were classified into three categories; wrong, partially correct, and correct answers. Concerning the test trials answers, the mean percentage of cor- rect (exactly similar or slightly idfferent from the actual response) responses per test trial (in case of the combined definitions, short, long, numerical and geometrical definitions) was used as a dependent variable. The types of definitions were arranged according to the dependent variable. Kendall rank order correlation coefficient was used to determine the correlation between the actual arrangement of the types of definitions and the theoretical arrangement which is predicted by paired-associate theory. Whenever the correlation was perfect, a second test was applied using 2 score to determine whether or not there was significant differences between any two prbportions of correct responses among the types of definitions. The same analysis was repeated using the mean percentage of the exact reSponses per test trial as a dependent variable. As for the pro-test answers, the X2 test was used to compare the pro-test score distributions of the levels of stimulus m, reSponse m, subject matter types, and length variables. Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar Also, the same approach.was repeated using post-test answers. In addi- tion, the X2 technique was used to test whether or not a significant difference existed between the pre-test scores and the post-test scores. The Results First: The distribution of definiendum m was bimodal, while the definition or definien m distributions were found approximately normal. The mean m of the definitions was found to be significantly higher than m of the response at the .01 level. However, the mean m of the defi- niendum was not significantly different from the mean m of the definien at the .05 level. Furthermore, m of numerical definitions was greater than m of the geometrical definitions at the .01 level. The results show a significant correlation between the m of geometrical terms and number of letters. There was significant correlation between the response m.and the number of words. The high positive correlations showed that the shorter the response or the geometrical term the higher was its m. 'While the intercorrelation coefficients between stimulus m or response m, and their corresponding lengths were not significantly different from zero at the .01 level, it was found that each one of these variables correlates significantly with definition m. The signi- ficant correlations between m of the definitions indicated that when definition m was high reaponse m was high, stimulus m was high and response number of words was few. The results showed that partialling any group of variables out of the correlation of definition m with other variables did not change the zero order correlation. However, once the definition m or other variables beside definition m.were par- tialled out, all the new correlations differed significantly from their Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar zero order correlation. These results emphasized the penetrating effect of definition m and its relation with reSponse m or stimulus m. ‘When definition m was partialled out the following results were obtained: (1) When reSponse m was high the reSponse was not neces- sarily high or low; (2) when response m was high the stimulus m was also high. Thus the effect of the variation in the number of letters in the stimuli, the variation in number of words in the reSponses, and the proved influence of definition m on stimulus as well as response m.indicates the presence of variables in definitions which are not usually studied in paired-associate learning. Second: The analysis of answers of the subjects in the test trials using the correct responses as a dependent variable were used to test both the theory which emphasizes the stimulus m, as well as the theory which emphasizes the response position. Each theory was tested fifteen times (with the combined definitions, long, short, numerical and geometrical definitions--per control, verbal and pic- ture familiarization). The theory which emphasizes the role of stimp ulus m was accepted twice in case of control and verbal familiariza- tion of numerical definitions. The theory which stresses the response m was accepted twice--for the control treatments with the combined and the long definitions. In each of the confirming cases there was some overlap among the types of definitions when both theories predict no overlap. Possibly the nature of the dependent variable was responsible for the failure to confirm either one of the theories with a high degree of consistency because verbal learning psychologists assume the exact reproduction of the response as their criterion measure of learning. Thus the general hypothesis proved to be untenable. Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar Third: When the exact response was used as a dependent variable, the results did not confirm the theory which favors the stimulus posi- tion. The theory which emphasizes the m of the reSponse was confirmed in three cases (combined, short and long definitions) with control treat- ment, and once (numerical definitions) with verbal familiarization. So the choice of the exact responses as a dependent variable improved the chances of confirming the general hypothesis. Fourth: The data concerning the familiarization conditions led to the rejection of the satiation hypothesis. Such deviation might have occurred as a result of using a familiarization procedure not com- pletely analogous to the familiarization procedures used by verbal learning psychologists. The proactive inhibition theory explained the results better than the satiation theory. Fifth: Analysis of pro-test scores showed that stimulus m.was more critical than response m. Thus definitions with higher m stimuli were better known in advance than definitions with lower m stimuli. The pre- and post-scores supported the hypothesis predicting an increase in definition attainment as an increasing function of stimulus m. Finally the analysis of post-test scores showed the presence of controversial findings between m of the stimulus and m of the response. While the arrangement of the types of definitions indicated that m of the stimulus was more critical in the post learning than response m, the statistical results were insignificant. These facts emphasized the difficulty in generalizing from the paired-associate model to definition learning. GENERALIZATION OF THE PAIRED-ASSOCIATE MODEL TO DEFINITION LEARNING By Salah Abdul-Moniem Hotar A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Educational Research, Measurement and Evaluation 1966 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The investigator wishes to express his deep gratitude and acknowledgements to Dr. Clessen J. Martin, Chairman of the Guidance Committee who assisted in the initiation and completion of this study and whose relationship has provided.professional growth and maturity. The other three distinguished members of the investigators' Guidance Committee: Dr. Benard Corman. Dr. Wilber Brookover, and Dr. Jay Artie have offered valuable suggestions. The investigator is indebted to Dr. Henry Kennedy, Dr. Clessen Martin, Dr. Leland Dean, and Dr. Clyde Dow, who convinced the princi- pals to participate in the experiment. Many principals, teachers and pupils have shown interest in this study. Their cooperation is appre- ciated. The investigator acknowledges the assistance of his colleagues: Mr. Rogers Bruce efficiently discussed the statistical procedure; Mr. Munir Morcos kindly drew the figures; Mr. Taha Hussein and Mr. Kamal El-Ganzoury have done considerable computer work. The investigator's wife, Nabilah, offered continuing support through her patience, understanding. and sacrifice. The investigator is grateful to her. The investigator and his wife extend their thanks to many American families. The families of the distinguished Professor Dr. Nelson Bossing, Dr. Irving Weyth, and Mr. James Brand made our stay in the United.States of America a happy and memorial experience. ii The investigator and his wife express their gratitude to their families and friends in the United Arab Republic (Egypt), eSpecially our fathers, Mr. Abdul Moniem Motwalley Hotar and Dr. Naguib Saleh Arwad, who gave us their encouragement. Finally, the investigator is greatly indebted to the people and the government of the United Arab Republic (Egypt). Their moral and financial support have carried us through these years of academic effort and achievement. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ii LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv LIST OF FIGURE 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 0 O O O O O O O O O O 0 Vi INTRDWCTION . O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 1 Related Literature 0 e e o o e o o e o e o o e e o e o a a 7 Significance of the Study and the Emerimnta]. Hypotheses O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 28 WHOD O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 36 Determination of Definitional Meaningfulness . . . . . . . 36 mermntd materifl-s O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 39 Design and Statistical Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . #5 REULTS . O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O 52 Definitional meaninnglneSS e e e e e o a a a a a e o e e a 52 Differential Effect of Meaningfulness of Definition Components on.Subjects' Learning . . . . . . 54 Subjects' Pre- and Post-Definition Attainment . . . . . . . 76 DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 BIEOIWRAPIH O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O 107 APPENDICE O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 112 iV' LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Methodolical Information Concerning the Reliability of the Meaningfulness Scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 2 Rank Order of the Mean Percentage of Correct ReSponses for the Types of Definitions in the Control Treatment. . 56 3 z Values for the Differences Between Proportions of Correct ReSponses in the Control Treatment . . . . . . . 58 4 Rank Order of the Mean Percentage of Correct Reaponses for the Types of Definitions in the Verbal Familiar- iz‘tion O O O O O C O C O C O O C O C 0 C O C O . O O O 61 5 z Values for the Differences Between Proportions of Correct Responses for Numerical Definitions With vorb‘l F‘miliarization I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C 63 6 Rank Order of the Mean Percentage of Correct Heeponses for the Types of Definitions in the Picture Familiarization . . . ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 7 Rank Order of the Mean Percentage of Exact Responses for the Types of Definitions in the Control Treatment. . 71 8 z Values for the Differences Between Proportions of Exact ReSponses for the Control Treatment . . . . . . . 72 9 Rank Order of the Mean Percentage of Exact Reaponses for Types of Definitions in the Verbal Familiarization . 73 10 z Values for the Differences Between Proportions of Exact Responses for Numerical Definitions With VerbalFamiliarization................o7’+ 11 Rank Order of the Mean Percentage of Exact Responses for Types of Definitions With the Picture Familiar- 1811:1011........................75 12 x2 Values of Pre- and Post-Tests' Scores With Definition Variables in Case of Control Treatment . . . . . . . . . 78 13 X2 Values of Pre- and Post-Tests' Scores for Types of Definitions Under Control Treatment . . . . . . . . . . 80 14 12 Values of Pre- and Post-Tests' Scores With Definition Variables in Case of Verbal Fandliarization . . . . . . 82 ‘v LIST OF TABLES (CONTINUED) Table Page 15 X2 Values of Pre- and Post-Tests' Scores for Types of Definitions Under Verbal Familiarization . . . . . . . 8h 16 12 Values of Pre- and Post-Tests' Scores with Definition Variables in Case of Picture Familiarization . . . . . 86 17 12 Values of Pre- and Post-Tests' Scores for Types of Definitions Under Picture Familiarization . . . . . . . 88 vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 The Percentage of Correct ReSponses Per Test Trial forcontrOlTreatmenteeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee59 2 The Percentage of Correct Responses Per Test Trial for Verbal aniarization e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 64 3 The Percentage of Correct Responses Per Test Trial for Pieture Familiarization e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 68 vii ZKHP‘: 0 LIST OF APPENDICES Page I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 112 eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee00113 O O O O O O O ........ O O I O O O O O O O O O O 114 O O O O O O O OOOOOOOOOOOOOO O C O O C O O 116 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 127 ..... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 0 O O O O O C O O O O 162 eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee163 eeeeeee eeeee eeeeeeeeeeeeeeee165 O O O O O O O O O ..... O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 166 eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee167 O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 169 O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 170 I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 171 O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O 172 eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeei73 O O O O O O O O O O C 0 O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O 17“ O O 0 O 0 O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O 175 O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O 176 viii LIST OF U1 0 O O O O O O O O O 02 O O O O O O O O O 0 v1 0 O O O O O O O O O APPENDICES (CONTINUED) Page .180 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181 O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 183 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Verbal learning psychologists are concerned with identifying the variables which affect learning. The general procedure involves the establishment of a miniature learning situation in which the learner is presented a number of verbal units for memorization. A wide range of variables has been studied within such learning situa- tions. The variables have involved manipulations of the meaningful- ness of the material, method of presentation. rate of presentation and a host of other such variables. The many possible manipulable variables have created a wide range of verbal learning experiments. Each one of them is fairly well controlled and designed to test Specific hypothesis concerning effects of the variables in question. However, this experimental approach has raised many objections. Learning psychologists (Deese, 1961; Rothkopf, 1963) claim that it tends to ignore or neglect verbal characteristics and skills in addi- tion to being restricted to examining elementary associations. Edu- cational psychologists have also objected to such learning situations as being too restricted and not applicable to meaningful materials. On the other hand, the advocates of this type of learning experi- ment believe that it stands squarely in the center of all human learn- ing. They claim that research in verbal learning creates phenomena and theories related to the study of the processes involved in the learning situation (Underwood. 196“). Such basic principles of learning make it possible to understand retention, forgetting, dis- crimination, generalization, transfer and problem solving behavior. Hilgard (1964) summarized the relationship between the latter points of view of learning theory and educational practices as that between any pure science and its technological applications. By pure science research, he meant that research which is guided by the problems which the researcher sets himself without regard for the immediate applicability of the results to practical situations. In applied research. on the other hand, the researcher is concerned with a practical problem directly relevant to classroom learning. The road from.pure science research to established educational practice has been classified into six steps. The first three of these steps relate to pure science research in learning and the others relate to the applied or technological research. The steps may be described as follows: Step 1. Research on learning with no regard for its educational rele- vance. e.g. animal studies. physiological, . . . Step 2. Research on learning which is not concerned with educational practices but which is more relevant than that of Step 1 because it deals with human subjects and with content that is nearer to that taught in school, e.g. nonsense syllable memorization and retention. . . Step 3. Research on learning that is relevant because the subjects are school-age children and the material learned is school subject matter or skill, though no attention is paid to the problem of adapting the learning to school practices, e.g. foreign language vocabulary learned by paired-associate method with various lengths of list and with various Spac- ing of trials. Step 4. Research conducted in Special laboratory classrooms, with selected teachers. . . . Step 5. A tryout of the results of prior research in a "normal" classroom with a typical teacher. . . Step 6. Developmental steps related to advocacy and adoption. Hilgard added that too much of the research in the past several decades has rested at Steps 1 and 2. Educational psychologists have tended to work at this end of the spectrum and then to jump, by inference, to Step 6, without being sufficiently patient about Steps 3, 4. and 5. He then emphasized the significance of the tasks to be done all along the six steps. Accordingly, the experiment reported in this study aims at being a kind of bridge between the two endpoints: Pure learning psychology and applied educational practices. The subjects are seventh grade school children, and the material consists of arithmetical defini- tions. The methodological approach is very similar to the laboratory methods of Hilgard's Step 2. However this study can be located in Hilgard's Step 3 where it is basically concerned with testing the validity of extending the existing theoretical notions in what is called paired-associate learning to the learning of the definitions. Verbal learning psychologists usually use nonsense syllables. two consonants separated by a vowel. in their experiments. These nonsense syllables are scaled according to their familiarity and ease of learning. This dimension of scaling is called "meaningfulness" (m). Pairs of verbal units are presented to the subject (S). The left hand member of the pair is designated as the stimulus term; the right hand member, the response term. The §fs task is to learn to be able to recall the reSponse term when the stimulus term is pre- sented alone. An example of a paired-associate unit is "LAJ-NOV" where "LAJ" is the stimulus term and "NOV" is the response term. Similarly, a definition is also composed of two parts, the definiendum and the definien, which are equivalent to the stimulus and response of a paired-associate. For example: "Face: A region of a plane enclosed by a polygon" is a definition.whose stimulus, i.e. definiendum, is the word "Face," and whose re5ponse, i.e. definien, is the phrase, "A region of a plane enclosed by a polygon." However, this similarity is not perfect and the differences between a paired-associate and a definition are numerous. (1) The components of a paired-associate might be nonsense syllables, unre- lated words, or letters. In the definition, the elements are mean- ingful words related contextually to each other. (2) The definition components are associated with a common meaning, while the components of a paired-associate unit are not necessarily related. (3) Almost all the studies in paired-associate learning use only one nonsense syllable with a constant number of letters as a reSponse while in the definition study the response is a phrase. Such definition responses usually vary in.length. For example, the two phrases: "The numbers zero and one," and "The process of finding how many times a number is contained in another number," are responses for the stimuli "Bigits" and "Division" respectively. While the first reaponse is composed of five words, the second is composed of fourteen words. (4) Stimuli' number of letters in paired-associate learning is a constant, that is each stimulus is composed of either two or three letters, but the num- ber of letters of the definition's stimulus is a variable. For example, "Pi" and "Multiplication" are stimuli of two arithmetical definitions. However each has a different number of letters. Because of these similarities and dissimilarities, it is difficult to make any generalization from paired-associate learning to definitions other than to suggest conservatively that there may be some correspondence between them. Again, the first objective of this study is to test the hypoth- esis that paired-associate learning can be used as a model for learn- ing definitions. The acceptance of this hypothesis requires two con» ditions (Brodbeck, 1963). First, there must be a one-to-one corres- pondence between the elements of a paired-associate and a definition. Second, certain relations must be preserved. That is to say we must know whether definition.learning is influenced by the same variables as paired-associate learning. The correspondence between a paired associate item and a definition is fairly obvious. The second condi- tion, i.e. that paired-associate and definitions are influenced by the same variables requires research. The variable selected for study and research in the present investigation involves the meaning- fulness (m) of the definiendum and definien. Then, the keystone is the coincidence of the findings of paired-associate experiments and the findings of the definition experiment with regard to the effects of m on learning. If manipulations in meaningfulness have similar effects on definition learning, then the correSpondence between this type of learning and paired-associate learning has been demonstrated with reapect to the m variable. The second objective of the present research is to test the appropriateness of the paired-associate model in relation to the findings on familiarization training. Verbal learning psychologists have studied the effect of pre-training or pre-differentiation on the differential effect of the stimulus and response members of a paired associate list. In the familiarization study, the material is pre- sented frequently and in advance of the learning trials for subjects. Teachers and textbook authors also try to familiarize their readers with either the definition stimulus, definition reaponse or both. HOWever, the educators' approach in familiarization is different from that of verbal learning psychologists. While the former tend to use ekartxples from experience, pictures, and models; the latter use fre- quent repetitions. For this reason, this study will introduce two C1ifferent methods of familiarization, verbal and picture explanation, which are similar to the educators' approach. The differential effect or verbal familiarization will be compared with that of picture familiarization. A third objective is to obtain the meaningflllness values for a number of arithmetical definitions. These values should have two advantages. First, they help to equate the experimental conditions of definition learning with the experimental conditions of paired associate learning. For example, to conduct an experiment in paired-associate learning, it is the usual procedure to choose a num- ber of nonsense syllables whose m values are predetermined and to relate the findings of the experiment to the level of the m values. But. in the case of definition learning the m values of either the stimuli, responses or definitions are not available. Therefore to make a comparison between the experimental findings of paired-asso— ciate learning and definition learning is questionable without con- trolling the m of definitions' stimuli and reSponses. Second, there are many elaborations on m of nonsense syllables. Verbal learning PSYChologists have done extensive work to understand, for example, the relationship between m of nonsense syllables and letter sequence. In definition learning while there is a lack of such studies, there is a. need to understand, the relationships between length of word (Stimulus), length of a sentence (response) and their correSponding “1 vGlues. Also there is a need to understand the relationship between the m of a sentence and the mean m of the vocabulary which forms the Sehtence. The knowledge of such relationships, and others to be reported, are of significance to the educational enterprise in gen— 81‘&1 and to understanding the results of this study in particular. RELATED LITERATURE The review of the literature will include: (a) The methods of scaling meaningmlness of the nonsense material. (b) The studies of the differential effect of the meaningful- ness of paired-associate components. (c) The studies of familiarization and its differential effect on meaningfulness of paired-associate components. Each part of the literature will include the theoretical consid- erations and the empirical studies. The Scalng of Meaningfulness The studies of verbal learning have shown that a verbal unit such as "MEX" will be learned much more rapidly than "XYJ." The dif- ferences between the rate of learning of the verbal units have been attributed to a factor called meaningmlness (m). The words which are easy to learn are supposed to have higher meaningfulness (m) Value than the words which are difficult to learn. Definitions of m h“’6 involved different approaches and sometimes different names. The earliest operational definition of m was based upon the munber of associates reported for an item in a certain fixed time. §3 Were asked to state in a word or phrase what the item meant for than. If the syllable meant something but the subject could not ”sprees its meaning within the time limit, he was to say "yes." The 1r'Qtal number of items presented ranged from 4534 to 320 per study, with the items presented by a tachistoscope or a memory drum for an e3l‘posure time which varied from two seconds to seven seconds. Prin- cipal contributors to this method were Glaze (1928), Hull (1933). KI'ueger (1934). Witmer (1935). Archer (1960), and Hilgard (1951). This m value has been designated as the association value. The second method of rating the m of the items is called the production method, devised by Noble (1952). and used by Mandler (1955). Meaningmlness was defined as the mean number of responses written during a certain time. However, the time in the production method is typically longer than the time for the association method. The maximum reported time using the production method is one hundred twenty seconds. The items were either dissyllables or nonsense syl- lables, presented on paper. The subject responded by writing all the different words elicited by the item, within a certain time. The third method of rating m has employed a rating scale. The subjects were asked to rate the item in terms of either ease of learning, familiarity, or pronunciation. This approach has been used by Haagen (1949), Noble, Stockwell and Pryer (1957), Underwood and Schulz (1960) and others. This m value has been referred to as the familiarity value. Goss and Nodine in 1965, called the association method and the Production method the single-association technique and the multiple- assOciation technique, respectively. Using the first method the "ulnber of association by a single subject to each stimulus may be limited to at most one association. With the second method the sub- ject may respond with as many associations as he can within inter- Vale. They called the scaling method as the experimenter-supplied stimuli for responses because each stimulus may be accompanied by One or more experimenter-supplied continuous stimuli in the form of graphic rating scales. Moreover. they assumed a direct relationship between frequency and m and that frequency can be considered as a basis for inferring m of stimuli. In this context frequency refers to the frequency of occurrence of stimuli as counted in samples of words in written texts. 10 The association, production and rating methods all appear to have some variance in common. The study reported by Mandler (1955) showed a correlation coefficient of .65 between the results of the production method and the number of associations method, using 100 syllables. Noble Stockwell and Pryer (1957) showed a correlation co efficient of .81 and .86 between the m values obtained by rating scale method and the values previously reported by Glaze and Krueger using the number of associations method for 100 syllables. Underwood and Schulz (1960) used Noble's items in three independent experi- ments. They found correlation coefficients between m values reported by Noble (who used the production method) on one side, and m values I‘eceived by scaling the item's ease of learning, familiarity and Pronunciation to be .90, .92, and .78. All the reported inter-corre- lation coefficients between the results of different techniques are sigl’iificantly different from zero. Rate of learning has been shown to be functionally related to In. Studies have shown that high m learning material is more readily learned than low m material. This relation is confirmed by the ex‘periments conducted by Lyon (1914), Reed (1929), Davis (1930), MeGeoch (1930), Sisson (1938), Noble (1952), Underwood and Richardson (1956), Dowling and Brown (1957), Sarason (1957). and Braun and Heyman (1958). The first two studies dealt with educational materials unscaled for m. The others had scaled items in the form of a serial learning task where the units were presented to the S in a constant order on each learning trial and he was required to learn them in the order presented. 11 Kimble and Dufort (1955), handler and Huttenlocher (1956), Noble and McNeeley (1957), and Noble, Stockwall and Pryer (1957) proved that the same relationship namely, that high m material is easier to learn than low m material, held in the case of paired-associate learning task where both components were of the same 111 value. An explanation was offered by Underwood (1949) that m of material facilitates learn- ing because of greater familiarity of such material. The previous studies have been concerned with scaling either nonsense syllables, nonsense figures, numbers, adjectives or nouns. There are no studies applying the concept of m for educational material, or even sentences. .___The Diffegential Effect of Meaniggi‘ulness of Paired-Associate Como- Lie nts on Legrning It has been shown that nonsense syllables can be scaled accord- ing to their m and that the higher the m, the easier the learning. This part of the literature survey will show the development of the tIMO-step theory and the empirical findings relevant to the role of the stimulus and response m on paired-associate learning. The two-step theory has been mentioned implicitly within the fI‘amework of information theory. Miller (1951) defined the amount of information conveyed by an item as dependent on the number of alternatives from which that item is chosen. For example, it is known that the twenty-six English letters occur with different rela- tive frequency, and it is possible to predict a letter in a word if the one or ones preceding it are known. However, the precision of prediction is dependent on the number of alternatives from which 12 the predicted letter is chosen. Given the letter "Q" in a word, then the only predicted letter to follow it is "U." The amount of infor- naaaision of "U" is dependent on one because it is the only alternative ifzrc>m which the choice is made. Again, given the letter "E" in a word, then to predict the letter following it is to make a choice from the other twenty-five alphabetic letters. Suppose that the letter which is chosen to follow the letter "E" is "N." The amount of information <>j?' "N" is then dependent on twenty-five, and is assumed to be greater i.r1 'value than the amount of information of "U" which has been shown to depend on one alternative. Information theory implies also that as the contextual con- straints increase, the information of the components of the verbal ‘1llist decrease. For example, in order to read the verbal unit "AEHV" one may tend to pronounce each letter individually because there is no previous learned context to integrate these letters, and the information per letter is high. On the other hand, the verbal unit as HAVE" is easier to pronounce. Here the constraint imposed by the aI‘rangement of the letters is high and the letters are not consid- ered independent entities. Rather, they are all components of one C30ntext, namely the familiar word "HAVE." Miller and Selfridge (1950) cited evidence that learning time iincreases with the amount of information communicated. By deduction, as contextual constraints increase the learning time is expected to decrease. Thus, in definition learning, the verbal unit "Repeated of: Process multiplication the addition," is more difficult than "Multiplication: The process of repeated addition." 13 Information theory so far has shown that the increase rate of learning for a verbal unit or a series of such units, is dependent on the degree of contextual constraints. However, the theory has not. placed a particular emphasis on paired-associate learning nor on the role of meaningfulness of its components. Hovland and Kurtz (1952) showed more explicitly than informa- tion theory that learning successive pairs of nonsense syllables involves two steps; one must not only learn the associations between the units, but also the units to be associated. This notion can also be applied in definition learning. In order to learn a definition such as "Point: A mathematical idea; associated with a location in Spaceflf the subject must learn the vocabulary of the definition, i.e. the words "Point, A, mathematical, . . ., Space," and then how to connect the stimulus "Point" with the response. H0Wover, the theory does not explain the steps involved in learning each unit of the paired-associate and associating them with each other. Handler (19510 presents three concepts in his proposal: (a) differentiating responses, (b) response integration, and (c) sym- holic responses. The first concept suggests that a stimulus is dif- 1‘ erentiated from other stimuli when it evokes a response different from one evoked by the other stimulus. This concept refers to the behavior of identifying and exploring the elements of the stimulus. The second concept is concerned with the elimination of subresponses which prevent or delay reinforcement. The third concept implies that any overt response which is perceived by a human organism evokes a 1Q symbolic response analogous to the overt reSponse. Mandler concluded that learning a response involves three steps: Differentiation, inte- gration and association through symbolic analogy. Underwood and Schulz (1960) have analyzed the acquisition of paired-associate or serial lists into two stages. The first stage is referred to as the reSponse learning or reSponse recall stage. In this stage the reSponse units are learned and connected to form a large unit. This step is similar to Mandler's (1951+) integration step. In the second stage, the associative or hook sup stage, the sub- ject connects the reSponse to a particular stimulus. Underwood and SChulz suggested that stimulus m exerts its effect on the associative StAge while the response m exerts its effect on response learning Stage. Hence, they concluded that the effect of stimulus m on paired asSociate learning is less than the corresponding effect of response me aningfulnes s . Two conditions must be fulfilled if paired-associate learning is to be considered a valid model for definition learning. One of these is that both the paired-associate and the definition must work on the same principle. Such principle was not clarified. Furthermore, the Paired-associate theories suggest the conclusion, that; m of the response member is more effective with respect to acquisition rate than m of the stinnllus'member. If the present experiment confirms this expectation, then paired-associate principles can be used for definition learning. The following portion of literature survey will show the find- ings of the empirical studies relevant to the effect of m on paired 15 associate learning. The earliest experiments used lists of paired-associates whose components could be classified roughly and as possibly having a high m (H) or a low m (L). The material consisted of an English word paired with a familiar word. The possible combinations were (1) H-H, high m stimulus and high m response; (2) L-H, low m stimulus and high m response; (3) H~L, high m stimulus and low m response; and (4) L-L, low m stimulus and low m response. These four combinations will be referred to in this study as pairedsassociate types. The lists might contain one type of pairedwassociate with many items, or four types of paired-associate (H-H, L-H, H-L, L-L) with very few items per type. Learning has been measured by the number of correct responses recalled directly after the end of the acquisition period. Stoddard (1929) asked a group of school children to learn from French words to English words, and the other group to learn from English words to French words. If it is considered that English words have higher m than French words, then this study provides a test of the relative influence of m of the stimulus versus m of the response. The mean test score for subjects who learned L-H (French- English) was 15.1 of 25, and for those who learned H-L (English- French) was 8.0 out of 25. Thus, it can be concluded that L-H pro- duced better learning than HAL and that high meaningfulness in the reSponse position is more critical than m of the stimulus position. Cason (1933) constructed 18 lists of 16 pairs each. The ver- b"Ll-units were familiar words (F) and.unfamiliar nonsense syllables (U) (F-F, U-F, F-U, and up). He had two groups of §_s, both of 16 which were given heterogenous lists of the types of familiar and unfamiliar verbal units. One group of §s was given the list to study for a period of four to eight minutes. The second group heard the pairs. An immediate recall test was administered in which the stimulus was pronounced and Spelled by the experimenter and the §s were to recall the response. Cason found that the two methods of study, auditory and visual, produced approximately the same results. Moreover, the recall of F-F (equivalent to H-H) was significantly greater than for U-U (L-L), but the U-F (L-H) and F-U (HAL) magnitude of recall was intermediate between F-F (H-H) and U-U (L-L). Sheffield (1946) used Cason's material in which various comp binations were preserved within the list, but the presentation was via the memory drum in order to control the time factor per unit. He demonstrated that H~H produced the most rapid rate of learning while L-L produced the slowest rate. The L-H and H-L rate of learn- ing was significantly different, contrary to Cason's findings. The L-H learning was slightly inferior to H-H, while H-L was slightly faster than L-L learning. Sheffield concluded that differences in m of the stimulus produces relatively minor changes in the rate of learning as compared with corresponding differences in the m of the reSponse. Kimble and Dufort (1955) prepared lists of ten paired-asso- CjHates in which the stimuli were ten items from Noble’s dissyllables. The ten dissyllables represented a complete range of m in Noble's sc:eil_e. Response terms consisted of common three-letter words. Thus, 17 it could be concluded that each list had these paired-associate types: H-H, H-L, and L-H. A group of'Ss learned the list with Noble's dis- syllables in the stimulus position, and a second group were given the same dissyllables in the reSponse position. Kimble and Dufort found that §s took more learning trials to anticipate correctly the list in which Noble's dissyllables were stimuli than when the dissyllables were reSponses. Cieutat, Stockwell and Noble (1958) formed the four combina- tions using Noble's dissyllables. Each list had only one combination. The H-L list and L-H lists were composed of identical items, but the positions were reversed. The lists were presented for twelve trials and each trial was followed by a test trial. Learning was measured in terms of the percentage of correct reSponses to each trial for each list. Their results showed the difficulty of learning increases in the order of H-H, L-H, H-L, and.L-L. Moreover, they found that varia- tion in stimulus m produced a much greater effect on learning when response m was low than when it was high; and that variation in reSponse m produced a much greater effect on learning when stimulus m was low than when it was high. The later results were confirmed by Lambert and Paivo (1956). Weiss (1958), L’Abate (1959). Hunt (1959), Underwood and Schulz (1960), Epstein (1963), Kothurkar (1963), Nodine (1963), Harleston (1963), Imartin, Cox and Boersma (1965), and G055 (1965). The generalization Of“the increase of learning trials in the order Huh, L-H, H-L, and L-L was found to hold under different experimental conditions whether the léiss1;s were administered to subjects singly or by a group technique. in 18 constant or varied order, under anticipation or recall formats and whether the subjects were college students or hospitalized mental patients. Thus, it is concluded that m of the reSponse members is from slightly to several times more potent than m of the stimulus member. Goss (1965) recently made an extensive review of the lit- erature on paired-associate learning and suggests that the available data are still too scanty either to account for exceptions to this generalization on rational grounds or to lead to more precise, reli- able generalizations about other patterns of factors. The previous emperical findings are consistent with the theo- retical notion which has been considered as the principle upon paired-associate learning works, namely, that m of the reSponse mem- ber is more reSponsible for the acquisition rate of a paired-associ- ate learning than m of the stimulus member. But it has not been shown whether such confirmed paired-associate theory is valid in case of definition learning. Then if it is proved that definien meaningfulness is more critical than definiendum meaningfulness the paired-associate model can be extended to include definition learn- ing. lbs Differential Effect of Familiggiggti2g_gfifigi;gd;§§§ggi§§g_ggflr Egnents on Learning The preceding literature has demonstrated that high m materials are‘ILearned faster than low m materials. One explanation of this PhehOmenon is that the high m material tends to be more familiar to the Subjects. Another explanation suggests that frequent experience witlkl the verbal material makes it more meaningful; thus its m DI 19 increases and is easier to learn. To test the relation between m, frequency of experience and rate of learning, psychologists have designed experiments in which verbal stimuli or responses are pre- sented to §s prior to the learning task. The purpose of such an experiment is to test the effect of such familiarization process on rate of learning. The usual method of familiarization is to require the Sp to repeat the stimulus to themselves (Gannon and Noble, 1961; Goss, Nodine, Gregory, Taub, and Kennedy, 1962), to repeat the stimulus aloud continuously for a certain time at a rela- tively high rate of repetitions per second (Lambert and Jackobvits, 1960; Kanungo, Lambert and Mauer, 1962), or to look at the stimuli for a period of time (Cieutat, 1960). These approaches to familiari- zation have been called either pre-training, pre-learning, or satia- tion. The role of familiarization has been explained by psychologists in relation to their theoretical framework. According to Miller's (1951) theory of information, familiarization reduces the number of alternate items from the range of all possible nonsense syllables. For example, the naive subject with the English language, given the letter "Q" to anticipate the second letter, responds with any letter, While the subject who is acquainted with this language will--with high .Probability--restrict his choice to the letter "U." The subject who is familiar with language structure is expected to restrict his responses to previously learned language habits. In addition, famil- iar‘1zation will tend to reduce the amount of information conveyed by each syllable at the time of learning. For example. before the 20 familiarization trials each word of the phrase, "The process of taking a number out of another number," is considered as unique and indepen- dent source of information by itself. The familiarization process helps the §_ to "chunk" the words together to form a smaller number of information units. So after familiarization one _S_ may perceive the phrase as composed of "The process of," "taking out of another," and "number." Another §_ might perceive the same phrase again as composed of smaller and smaller units. Gibson (1940) has mentioned that the familiarization procedure produces discrimination between the units to be associated and those learned in earlier lists. She predicted two types of errors to be reduced: Interlist and "invention" errors. This prediction is con- sistent with Miller's theory of reducing the number of alternatives to the limited number which have been offered in the familiarization trials. For example: If a §_ is presented frequently with a list of geometrical definitions to become familiar with, and the same list is given to the same S for learning, then the erroneous responses that are expected to be reduced are those involving recalling a numerical response or inventing a haphazard answer for the geometrical stimulus. Gibson also predicted a reduction of intralist errors if the §_ is familiarized with the learning material. Thus, it can be assumed that subjects who are given a list of geometrical definitions for familiarization and then for learning are less likely to attach a geo- metrical response to a different geometrical stimulus. Hovland and Kurtz (1952) were able to show that familiarization enables the sub- Jec‘t to recognize the interlist and invention errors but does not 21 reduce the intralist errors. Mandler's (1954) view is that with successive repetition of a response aggregate, the separate reSponses eventually become stimuli for each other in a way that any part of the reSponse aggregate will tend to evoke the whole reSponse. This is designated as an integra- tion process and its growth is dependent upon elimination of responses which prevent or delay reinforcement. Still Handler's explanation is directed towards the response and, in a sense, he views the famil- iarization process as responsible for limiting the number of alter- natives and making such correct responses more integrated. Underwood and Schulz (1960) added that familiarization is a procedure for making reSponses more available during subsequent association learning. Then, it could be concluded from these theoretical notions that familiarization makes two contributions: First, it reduces the number of alternatives to the learned ones; second, it reduces the separate information elicited by the components of the familiarized item, and thus makes it integrated and more available during learning. While the preceding psychologists emphasize that familiarization facilitates learning of low meaningfulness items, there are others who state that it has a prohibitive effect, especially if the material is of high meaningfulness. Theadvocates of the latter theory are Lambert and Jakobvits (1960), Kanungo, Lambert and Mauer. (1962), and Kanungo and Lambert (1963). Their explanation is in terms of either meaning decrement or the development of a word-word habit. The first explanation suggests that too much repetition for the H material causes its m value to decrease, and the material becomes judged as L. 22 Since high m material facilitates learning and low m material retards learning, the items which were originally H become L. Therefore, familiarization of high m material may actually produce a decrement in learning. However, the same theory states that the familiariza- tion of low meaningfulness material makes it high m and so its avail- ability and ease of learning will increase. In the second explanation, researchers claim that familiariza- tion increases the number of "books" or associations of the item, and so the chance of associating the verbal unit with other verbal units increases. As for the L verbal unit, its number of associates will increase and thus will be more readily associated with other new items. On the other hand, the familiarization of H also increases its number of associates, but these associates will be used to tie this H item with itself rather than with another item. Hence the number of asso- ciates are assumed to be extinguished in developing word-word habits. For example, the familiarization of the dissyllable "GOJEY" makes it better integrated, develops its number of hooks and thus increases its availability for association with any other item. On the other hand. the familiarization of a well integrated verbal item as "KITCHEN" which already has many hooks will create several items as "Kitchen, kitchen, . . ." and each one of them is similar to "KITCHEN" in terms Of'the number of hooks. But the hooks of each item will be associated tC> the hooks of other items, and all of them will be consumed in devel- oping a word-word unit such as "Kitchenkitchen" in that no other hooks 31‘ e left to be associated to another new verbal unit. Therefore there 23 is less chance for familiarized H verbal item to be recalled as if it were an L. The theory predicts, once more, that the paired—associate types H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L, on familiarization with the stimuli, responses, or both, will have an acquisition rate similar to L-H, H-H, L-L, and H-L, or H-L, L-L, H-H, and L-H, or L-L, H—L, L-H, and H-H reSpectively. This prediction is built on the basis that the paired-associate member which is either H or L and is given familiarization will turn conse- quently to be L or H. Another aspect of familiarization can be inferred from Postman and Phillips (1964) empirical findings. They observed that the rela- tionship between amount of recall and degree of contextual constraints to be curvilinear and concluded that when material context is highly constrained. recall is difficult, as in the case of unstructured material. In addition recall is relatively easier when contextual constraint is neithefhigh nor low. Although they reported this relationship using Miller's (1950) terminology, the role of informa- tion theory was not clarified in explanation of their findings. How- ever, using this observation, it is possible to explain the relation- ship between familiarization and contextual constraint, and between contextual constraint and recall behavior. It can be argued that as familiarization increases, contextual constraints increase, and ease of recall is then determined by the degree of the imposed constraints. Taking into consideration the curvilinear relationship, the familiari- zation of an L material adds a moderate contextual constraint, and accordingly its recall will be easier than before familiarization 24 trials. But the familiarization of an H material increases the pre- vious imposed contextual constraint and then makes it difficult to recall. The following section will review the findings of the studies relevant to the effect of familiarization on m in serial and paired associate learning. Solomon and Postman (1952) controlled experimen- tally the frequency of usage of Turkish words by asking subjects to read and pronounce them with frequencies ranging from 1 to 25. They found that recognition thresholds varied inversely with frequency of prior usage. Noble (1954) offered 18 L items to 288 college students in a serial form with different frequencies. He obtained a close relation between the judged familiarity and frequency of occurrence. Arnault (1956) using nonsense shapes came to a similar conclusion to that of Noble (1954), namely that m and familiarity are closely related, doubtless as a consequence of the number of previous famil- iarization trials. The curves representing these relationships are negatively accelerated between zero and 40 acquisition trials, and diminish rapidly around the twentieth trial. On the other hand, Lambert and Jackobvits (1960) feund that semantic satiation reliably moves the rating of the term towards the meaningless point of the scale. Kanungo, Lambert and Mauer (1962) feund that satiation treat- ment caused a decrease in the connotative meaning of words receiving many familiarization trials. Another criterion used to measure the effect of familiarization, other than the judged familiarity, is the number of learning trials required to learn the material. Hovland and Kurtz (1952), Noble (1955). 25 Rilqy and.Phillip (1959) and Underwood and Schulz (1960), found a significant reduction in the number of trials required for mastery of serial tasks as a result of the level of familiarization. Familiarization of L material has been found to affect the rate of learning in ways similar to that of m in paired-associate learning. It is expected. according to the theories, that greater facilitation would result when the pro-learned unit appeared as the response in the paired-associate than as the stimulus member. This means that the L response member may become an H response. For example, in a list of’LeL items, in which the L reaponse member has been.prelearned, then the list would be similar to L-H list. Again, if the stimulus member of the list LAL receives familiarization, the list becomes similar to HAL list. Hence, the arrangement of the lists according to their theoretical ease of learning is as follows: Unfamiliarized L-familiarized.L, familiarized L-unfamiliarized.L, and unfamiliarized.L-unfamiliarized.L. The theories which suggest this order are, first; the two step theory which emphasizes the role of m of response member over the m of the stimulus member in paired-asso- ciate learning and, second; the familiarization theory which predicts that L material will become equivalent to H material through the pre- learning trials. Goss (1965) reported that Scheffield (1946) compared the acqui- sition of an H-H list without response familiarization and an HéL list with response familiarization. He found that familiarization of response members of H-L list was facilitative. Weiss (1958) com. pared the acquisition of H-H, HAL, and.L-L with familiarized reSponses, 26 and unfamiliarized reSponses. He found that mean trials to criterion adjusted for practice performance were lower with familiarized reSponse members than with unfamiliarized reSponse members. However there are studies which have found no significant facili- tation when reSponse members are familiarized. Cieutat (1960) used two mixed lists of four pairs of L dissyllables with one pair repre- senting each of the four combinations of familiarization and unfamil- iarization. The same subjects were used in all the treatments. Familiarization was by looking at the familiarized items for sixty seconds. He found that familiarization with the response member inhi- bits learning with an unfamiliarized stimulus member, and is facilita- tive with a familiarized stimulus member. The arrangement of the combinations according to ease of learning was familiarized-familiar- ized, unfamiliarizedaunfamiliarized, familiarized-unfamiliarized and unfamiliarized-familiarized. Neither familiarization of stimulus mem. bers nor familiarization of response members had a significant effect. Such unexpected arrangement of the results might be due to the use of a mixed list, and a few number of items to represent each combination. Moreover, using the same subjects for learning all combinations might have made them more experienced and more selective over the entire task. Another study which showed that familiarization of response mem- ber did not improve learning was that of Kanungo, Lambert and Mauer (1962). They formed a paired-associate list identical with H-H, using high frequency words. Two groups learned this task, and one of them obtained semantic satiation for the reSponse member. Those with 27 response satiation were inferior in the learning. Kanungo and.Lambert (1963) showed that, with an H-H list, semantic satiation of either the stimulus or the reSponse words retards subsequent learning. They explained their results in terms of the m of the members of the paired associate and the locus of familiarization. Concerning the familiarization of the stimulus member, the results are not conclusive. The findings of Gannon and Noble (1961), Martin (1963), and Schulz and Martin (1964) support the idea of famil- iarization having a facilitative effect when the stimulus member was familiarized. Other studies reported that such familiarization would produce an inhibitive effect. For example, Neiss (1958) compared acquisition of familiarized and unfamiliarized stimulus members of the following paired-associate types: H-H, L-H, and L—L. He found that mean trials to criterion, adjusted for practice performance, were lower with familiarized stimulus members than with unfamiliarized stimulus for H-H and L-H, but not for LAL combination. The results of Weiss, could be reported differently if the m of the stimulus had been considered. One may conclude that H stimuli became L, and vice versa on stimulus familiarization of the H-H and L-L. The conclusion of prohibitive effect of the familiarized stimuli, explained as a result of having H items, was mentioned in.Kanungo, Lambert and Mauer (1962) and Kanungo and Lambert (1963). Finally. a study by Bailey and Jeffrey (1958) reported no sig- nificant effect for pro-learning in either member of the paired-asso- ciate. They asked.§s to learn three successive lists of paired non- sense syllables in which the stimulus term was different in each list 28 but the response term remained the same. In the test list the reSponse terms were paired.with syllables. The familiarized syllables were in either the stimulus position or in the response position. The test list learning of these pairs under either condition did not differ from learning under control conditions. Using a number of pre-learning trials that were insufficient to produce significant differences between the treatments might be reSponsible for this result. The previous results are not consistent. These studies have employed different levels of m, different familiarization procedures, and different learning procedures. Such differences may well intro- duce unspecified variables that make agreement among all the results an impossibility. However, to make a better prediction or explana- tion, it is necessary to know the meaningfulness of each member of the paired-associate before familiarization, the locus of familiarization relevant to the familiarized and the control treatments. SIGNIFICANCE OF THE STUDY AND THE EXPERIMENTAL HYPOTHESES A gap has existed between verbal learning psychologists and edu- cators. Verbal learning psychologists are concerned with studying the basic mechanisms of learning by using nonsense syllables in rigid experimental procedures with adult subjects. However, the educators have been concerned whether the material, the method, and even the findings have any objective applications to the classroom. Accord- ingly, the present study stands between these two extremes and attempts to test the appropriateness of extending the paired-associate model to 29 definition learning. Thus, the major purpose of this study is to test the hypothesis that paired-associate learning can be used as a model for learning arithmetical definitions. The variable which has received the greatest attention among verbal learning psychologists has been the meaningfulness of the material. Many studies have been concerned with different methods of scaling the m of these materials. Such material has involved either nonsense syllables, nonsense figures, dissyllables, or num- bers. However the review of the literature failed to find any edu- cational material which has been scaled for m. Therefore one aspect of this study is concerned with determining the m of a num- ber of arithmetical definitions as well as the m of the individual definiendum and definien. These m values make it possible to inquire about some relation. ships which have not been studied in paired-associates. For example, in this definition study it is possible to determine the interrela- tionships between m of definitions, m of definiendum, m of definien, number of letters in the stimuli, and number of words in the response. By the use of partial correlation coefficient techniques it is pos- sible to determine which variables effect the m of a definition. Also it is possible to determine the relationship between m of the entire definition and the summed m values of its components. How- ever, while these relationships are important in understanding the factors which affect the m value of definitions, they are of minor concern in this study. 30 The verbal learning theoreticians are in agreement that m is dependent on familiarity and frequency of experience. They explain the ease of learning H material as due to its familiarity and avail- ability fOr recall. In learning a paired-associate item, the learner tends to integrate the smaller units of the response to produce one which is more available and ready to be associated with the stimulus member. On the other hand, in recalling the response member of a paired-associate, the learner tries to limit his answers to the learned items and tends to recall the well integrated reaponses better than the unfamiliar or unavailable reSponses. Because of the reasonable consistency between empirical find- ings and paired-associate theory it is concluded that m of the response member of a paired-associate has a greater effect upon learn- ing than m of the stimulus. Hence, if it can be demonstrated that definition learning is influenced by the same variables as paired associate learning, then the paired-associate model can be genera- lized to definition learning. Paired-associate research has yielded contradictory results con- cerning the effect of familiarization on either the stimuli or reSponses prior to the actual learning task. Psychologists have dif- ferent explanations, but they have emphasized the role of the meaning- fulness of the familiarized material more than its position. The data suggest that familiarization of low meaningfulness members makes them more integrated and changes them to readily available, highly meaning- fulness members. On the other hand, familiarization of high meaning- fulness items tends to produce a kind of satiation of meaning. 31 In order to test the hypothesis that the mechanism underlying the learning of arithmetical definitions is similar to the mechanism involved in paired-associate learning, it is necessary that the experimental situations remain as similar as possible. The m values which have been obtained for the arithmetical definition stimuli and responses, permit manipulation of the four basic types of definitions: H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L. Each type of definition is represented by four arithmetical definitions. For example in case of L-H, two of them have relatively short definiens (reSponses), and the other two have long definiens (responses). One of the short definitions is numerical and the other short definition is a geometrical term. Simi- larly, in case of definitions with long responses, one of them is numerical and the other is geometrical. This study will test the hypothesis that definition types, arranged according to their ease, are similar to the paired-associate types when arranged in accordance with the response theory. However, the empirical rank order of definition types will be correlated with the suggested order of the theory which stresses the importance of the stimulus position and the theory which favors the importance of the reSponse position. Very little theoretical attention has been given to the role of stimulus m in paired-associate learning. Theo- retical attention has been focused almost exclusively on the role of response m. However, this study will attempt to assess the role of StiJnulus m as well as reSponse m. It will be possible to assess the effkacts of m on the stimuli and responses in a number of different insflances. The effect of m will be assessed among long and short 32 definitions, numerical and geometrical definitions, and the combined definitions within a list. Concerning familiarization, this study will investigate the effects of verbal and picture familiarization. This method of famil- iarization is contrasted with the type of familiarization procedures used in standard paired-associate learning tasks. The standard familiarization procedure typically involves frequent repetitions of the material prior to the actual learning tasks. However the famil- iarization procedures employed in this investigation are more similar to actual classroom practice. In addition, familiarization is devoted almost entirely to the response member of the definition. On the basis of paired-associate familiarization data it is assumed that familiarization of a high m verbal unit reduces the m value of that unit and familiarization of a low m unit increases the m value. If it is assumed that stimulus m is more influential in learning than is the reSponse m, then before familiarization the following rank order would be H-H, H-L, L-H, and L-L. But if famil- iarization has the effect of extinguishing high m values, then the order as a result of familiarization would be H-L, H-H, L-L, L-H. However, if reSponse m is more influential in learning than stimulus m, then prior to familiarization the order of difficulty would be H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L. But this order would be expected to change to H-L, L-L, H-H, and L-H as a result of reSponse familiarization. The correlations between these predicted rank orders and the actual rank orders will be used as a basis for determining the generality of the paired-associate model to definition learning. 33 Another aspect of this study is concerned with the extent the § has actually learned to state the definition in his own words. This measure is referred to as definition attainment. The emphasis here is not upon verbatim repetition of the exact words and phrases. However the §_is encouraged to put the meaning of definition's stimuli in his own words before and after the learning trials. It is believed that m of the stimulus position is more concerned with definition attainment because the connotation of definition meaning is represented in the stimulus member. The exact response is an arbitrary arrangement of words defining the stimulus. In other words, the meaning of the definition is represented by its stimulus and can be expressed in dif- ferent ways or in different verbal arrangements. When the subject is asked about the meaning of a familiar stimulus, he can reSpond cor- rectly in different ways, e.g. by explaining the stimulus, giving an example, or making an analogy. This study will also compare definition attainment before and after the learning task, and attempt to state whether the m of the stimulus or the response member is more important in increasing defi- nition attainment. Experiment§l_Hypotheses A. Analysis of test trials. The control treatments: 1. The increase of m of response member of the definition will be accompanied by an increase of mean percentage of correct responses per test trial. The arrangement of the types of definitions from superior to inferior, The 1. 3“ with respect to the mean percentage of correct responses per test trial is as follows: H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L. The H-H, L-H, H-L, L-L arrangement will also exist if the dependent variable is the mean percentage of exact reSponses per test trial. The stated arrangement of 1 and 2 will hold in case of using the entire task, short, long, arithmetical and geometrical definitions. verbal and picture familiarization treatment: Verbal familiarization of the response member will be accompanied by either an increase or decrease of the mean percentage of either the correct or the exact reSponses per test trial. The increase will occur if the familiarized reSponse is high in meaningfulness, and the decrease will occur if the familiarized response is low in meaningfulness. The arrangement of types of definition, according to the response theory, from superior to inferior with respect to the percentage of correct or exact reSponses, is expected to be in this order: H-L, L-L, H-H, and L-H. Picture familiarization will produce the same order of arrangement as verbal familiarization. The stated order of 1 and 2 in case of familiarization, will hold using the entire task, short, long, arithmeti- cal and geometrical definitions. 35 B. Analysis of pre- and post-definition attainment scores. 1. The increase of stimulus m will be accompanied by an increase of both pre- and post-test scores. But the increase of response m will not be accompanied by any increase in either the pre- or post-test scores. There will be a significant difference between the distributions of pre- and post-test scores. The relationship stated in 1 and 2 will hold in case of control, verbal or picture familiarization conditions. Under each familiarization treatment the significant differences between the distribution of pre- and post- test scores will decrease in the following order: H-H, H-L, L-H, and L-L. CHAPTER II METHOD DETERMINATION OF DEFINITIONAL MEANINGFULNESS Arithmetical Definition Four arithmetical textbook series were used to gather arithmeti- cal definitions which appear in grades five, six, seven and eight. The names of the textbooks are given in Appendix A. Definitions with stimuli composed of more than one word (e.g. "square root," "right angle") or with symbols in the reSponse (e.g. "an angle which measures more than 90°," "any number that can be named by a fraction of the form a/b, where a and b are integers, with the restriction that b is not 0") were excluded. Ninety-seven definitions were finally used in this part of the study. Forty-eight of these definitions can be represented in numbers and are called numerical definitions. The other forty-nine can be represented by graphs and are referred to as geometrical definitions. The two kinds of definitions, numerical and geometrical, were randomly arranged. The stimuli and the responses were separated from each other and the m value of each was determined separately by the use of two rating scales. Meaningfulness was defined operationarby in terms of the students' judged familiarity and ease of learning. The first page of each scale contained the instructions and examples. The other pages of the rating scale included the rated items and m continuum based on five points: Very easy, easy, 36 37 indifferent, difficult, and very difficult. These pages of the rating scale were presented in different random orders. A copy of the instruction page is shown in Appendix B. Subjects, Procedure, and Reliabilities TABLE 1: Methodological Information Concerning the Reliability of the Meaningfulness Scales Information ' Stimulus Response Reliability of AD .9813 .9033 Reliability of ND .9847 .9ouu Reliability of’GD .9778 .8620 Reliability Method Test Retest Test Retest No. of Students 30 30 M . Age in Months 145 145 Grade 6 6 School B B Dates of Administrating 5/7/65 5/7/65 the Scale 5/11/65 5/11/65 AD = Arithmetical definitions ND Numerical definitions GD = Geometrical definitions Table 1 contains the number of students who volunteered in rating the m of each scale, their median age, their grade and their school. It also includes the dates of administering the different rating scales. 38 The experimenter used a standard procedure in administering the rating scales. First, the §s were asked to read the printed instruc- tions. Second, the experimenter explained the instructions verbally (see Appendix C) and, using the blackboard, showed how to indicate the ratings for the examples given. All questions asked by the §s were answered, but the experimenter did not give a definite rating to the examples. Third, the Se were asked to read all the items before rating them. Fourth, the Ss were asked to reread each item carefully and to check the appropriate point on the scale. In order to determine the meaningfulness and standard deviation for each scaled item, the points very easy, easy, indifferent, diffi- cult and very difficult were assigned the weights one, two, three, four and five respectively (following Thurstone and Chave, 1929). Stimuli and responses' m values were checked for reliability by administering the rating scale twice. Therefore, for each stimulus or response two m values and two standard deviations were obtained. A third m value was computed.by pooling the students‘ two ratings. This m value and its corresponding standard deviation are referred to as the pooled values. The pooled m value and the pooled standard deviation are considered the standard values for each item. The reliability coefficients and the method used in estimating them are shown in Table 1. All of the reported coefficients are above .90 with the exception of the reliability of geometrical responses which is .862 and all are significantly different from zero (P< .01). 39 Appendix D contains the m values of the rated material. It should be remembered that the lower the value of m, the higher the meaningfulness of the item. This has resulted from assigning ascend— ing weights to descending ease and familiarity of the rating scale points. EXPERIMENTAL MATERIALS Selection of Definitions The stimulus m of each item was paired with its corresponding reSponse m. Thus each definition had two m values, one for its stimp ulus and one for its reSponse. Four lists of definitions were chosen. Each set contained four definitions, two numerical and two geometrical. One definition of each kind was long and the other was short. However, each list had approximately the same total number of words. The H-H list of definitions contained high m values for both its stimuli and its reSponses. Its m value for the stimuli ranged between 1.153 and 1.678. The m.value for the reSponses ranged between 1.864 and 2.271. In the L-H list the stimuli's m values ranged between 3.983 and 4.390 while the responses ranged between 1.915 and 2.542. The H-L list consisted of stimulus m values between 1.034 and 1.898 and response m values between 2.593 and 3.898. The L-L list of definitions contained stimulus values between 3.475 and 4.559 and response values between 2.695 and 3.898. Appendix G presents the selected items. 4O Familiarization Materials The materials used in the familiarization process were of two types, verbal and picture familiarization. Verbal familiarization materials were prepared as follows. The experimenter prepared three different written explanations for each selected definition. Each explanation was composed of three sentences. The three explanations were attached to the reSponse part of the definition. Those responses and their corresponding explanations were given to three graduate students who were told that one out of every three explanations would be presented to sixth grade students. They were asked to rate each explanation according to its appropriateness for sixth grade students, by rank ordering the three explanations from "highly appropriate" to "less appropriate." The ranks were summed for each explanation. For each response, the explanation with the lowest sum of ranks was chosen as the verbal familiarization material for the corresponding definition. In case of picture familiarization, the selected definitions were classified according to whether they were numerical or geomet- rical definitions. The numerical definitions were explained by pre- senting three number operations, while the geometrical definitions were explained by drawing three consecutive pictures. The same graduate students were requested to rank order the three number opera- tions or consecutive pictures per definition response, with the same instructions as previously. The selected picture familiarization Imaterials were those explanations which had the lowest sum of ranks. .A11.the familiarization materials are shown in Appendix H. 41 Subjects Four hundred and thirty-four volunteer students enrolled in the seventh grade were used as Ss. Their median age was 151 months at the time of the experiment. Appendix I shows the number of students assigned to each task. Procedure The learning task was administered by the use of a group proce- dure. Each group of’Ss was assigned at random to one of the treat- ments. The students were given a test booklet containing eight pages. The first page provided space in order to obtain information about the st sex, birthdate, and name. The other seven pages contained only the stimuli with blank Spaces where the‘Ss wrote their recalled reSponse beside each stimulus. The stimuli were arranged randomly on each page. The definitions as well as the familiarization material were placed on thermofax transparencies and were presented manually by an overhead projector. The thermofax transparencies of the definitions were covered by two separate pieces of paper, one of which covered the stimulus and the other the response. The two covers were used to pro- ject separately either the stimulus or the response of the definition. During the learning trials, the Sp were shown each stimulus for approximately five seconds, followed by the stimulus and the response for approximately fifteen seconds. The total time needed for each learning trial per definition was about twenty seconds. Since there were four definitions per task, each learning trial required eighty seconds. The time required for one test trial was three minutes and thirty seconds. In case of familiarization, the experimenter took 42 four minutes to present the familiarization material. Presentation rates were determined by the use of a stop watch. The experimenter followed the steps shown below while he was conducting the experiment. First, the §s were instructed to turn to the second page of the booklet and to give the meaning of each word (stimulus). This pre-test was for the purpose of determining the extent to which the definition had been attained prior to the learn- ing task. Second, if the treatment included familiarization, the experimenter showed the §s the corresponding familiarizations and the experimenter helped them by reading the material aloud. Third, the experimenter showed the §s one of the stimuli, asked them to read it, and anticipate its meaning before reading the projected sentence (response) within the time limit which has been mentioned before. The third step involved the presentation of all four definitions. One complete presentation of all definitions is referred to as a learning trial. Fourth, in the test trials the §s were asked to write down the exact sentence (response) which had been shown to them with its cor- reSponding word (stimulus). There were nine learning trials (1) and five test trials (t) arranged in this order: l-ti, ll-tz, ll-t3, ll-tu, ll-ts. Fifth, the §s were asked to express the meaning of each stimu- lus using their own words. The post-test was to determine definition attainment after the learning task. Treatments It has been stated previously that there were four different types of definitions: H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L. There were also three kinds of familiarization. The §s received either no familiarization, verbal 43 familiarization or picture familiarization. Those §s who did not receive familiarization were designated as a control group. The combination of the four types of definitions with the three kinds of familiarization produced twelve treatment conditions. Each treatment was identified by the type of definition and the kind of familiarization. The treatments are designated by combining the sym. bols of the type of definition with the symbols referring to the type of familiarization. For example, H-H refers to definitions with high m stimuli and high m reSponses when presented without familiarization, while L-H P designates definition with low m stimuli and high m reSponses presented with picture familiarization. Dependent Variables The Ss' answers in the test trials were classified.ixto eight dif- ferent categories. It was assumed that these categories represented a continuum which started with a "no answer," and ended with "recalling the exact reSponse," and covered the different levels of the answers. Each category was labeled to explain its common property and was assigned a score depending on its location within the continuum. A sample of the Ss' answers to the stimulus "abscissa" will be presented to explain the scoring system. As mentioned in Appendix E, this stimulus is one of L-H definition and its response is "the dis- tance measured horizontally to a point." Other stimuli which are found in the L-H type of definitions are "predecessor," "uniqueness" and "hypotenuse" and their responses are shown in the same appendix. The latter reSponses are of concern to the response of the stimulus "abscissa" since some Ss confused and/or mixed these responses with 44 the "abscissa" response in their test trials. The scoring system and a sample of the answers to the stimulus "abscissa" are shown below. 1. No answer: Assigned for the answers which were left blank or if the §_wrote "I do not know" or "A distance" i.e. when the reported number of words was less than one quarter of the exact response. 2. Outside or inventional answer: Answers which were comp pletely unrelated to any response in the whole task as "A line drawn to the center of a circle." 3. Confused answer: Response of one stimulus given to another stimulus as "There is only one sum correct the sum of the number." 4. Mixed answers: §_mixed two different reSponses and responded with this mixture to a given stimulus as "A number measured horizontally." 5. Between one quarter and one half of the exact response: e.g. "The distance measured." 6. One half of the exact response but less than three quarter: e.g. "The distance measured horizontally." 7. Three quarters of the exact resggnse, or the exact response written in a different form: e.g. "The distance horizontally to a point." 8. The exact resEonse: As "The distance measured horizontally to a point."_ Studies in paired-associate learning have been concerned with measuring the exact responses. But the strategy of this study requires a more flexible scoring system. Thus limiting the analysis to the 45 exact responses (category 8) would exclude other answers which were almost correct (category 7). Categories seven and eight have been combined and classified as correct responses. The mean percentage of these correct responses is a dependent variable. This study also compared the §fs mastery of the definitions before and after the learning trials. For this reason the answers of the pre- and post-tests have been classified into three categor- ies. The assigned scores and the types of answers are shown below. 0. No answer, outside answer, confused answer or mixed answers. 1. Answers which were partially correct. 2. Correct answers. The use of these categories permitted an assessment of defini- tion attainment as a result of the learning trials. The dependent variable in this part of the analysis was the given score per defini- tion in the pre- and post-tests. DESIGN AND STATISTICAL PROCEDURE The available variables are definition m, stimulus m, response m, number of letters in the stimulus and number of words in the response. These five variables are presented in Appendices D and H. The first description of these variables is in terms of their means and standard deviations. Second, analysis of variance has been utilized to test the effects of subject matter levels and definition components on meaningfulness. In making this test, the fixed-effect model for two-factor completely randomized design was applied. Third, the intercorrelation coefficients among these five variables have been 46 calculated in order to determine the interrelationships among them. Moreover the contribution of each definition variable to its m is shown by partialling out one or more of these variables. The partial correlations are compared to the zero order correlation coefficients, and the results of these analyses are presented in some detail in Appendix F. The twelve treatments are produced by having four types of defi- nitions (H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L) and three kinds of familiarizations (control, verbal and picture). Each §_received five test trials in which to recall the responses of the four stimuli. Bartlet's test has been applied to test the homogeneity of test trial variances. The test revealed a X2 value of 402.7485 which is significant at the .01 level. The hypothesis concerning the homogeneity of test trials vari- ances for the twelve treatments was rejected and the presence of heter- ogenous variances suggested the use of non-parametric methods in data analyses. The mean percentage of correct responses per test trial is used as a dependent variable. Appendices J, K, and L show the mean percen- tage of correct responses per test trial as well as the mean percentage of such correct responses in case of the combined definitions, and of short, long, numerical and geometrical definitions, under the three different familiarization.procedures. The types of definitions (H-H, L—H, H-L, L-L) of any of the fifteen treatment conditions can be arranged according to the mean percentage of correct responses. The rank order of the types of definitions suggested by the theory emphasizing the m of the reSponse member is H-H, L-H, H-L, LAL 47 without familiarization, and H-L, L-L, H-H, L-H with response familiari- zation. On the other hand, the suggested arrangements of the types of definitions by the theory which emphasizes the m of the stimulus posi- tion is H-H, H-L, L-H, L-L without familiarization, and HAL, H-H, LAL, L—H with response familiarization. The logic behind these arrangements has been explained in the first chapter of this study. Kendall rank order correlation coefficients were used to deter- mine the correlation between the actual arrangement of the types of definitions as reported in Appendices J, K, and L, and the arrangement which is suggested by either the theory emphasizing the role of m in the response, or the theory which emphasizes the role of stimulus meaningfulness. Separate Kendall rank order correlations were computed in order to test the two theories relating to m of the stimuli and responses in relation to the various tasks (combined, short, long, numerical and geometrical definitions), and different types of famil- iliarization (control, verbal, and picture familiarization). Whenever the actual arrangement of the types of definition, under any one of the familiarization conditions, correlated perfectly with any of the suggested theoretical orders, a second test was applied to determine whether or not there were significant differences between any two proportions of correct responses among the types of definitions. The 2 score, and the normal distribution table were used to test whether the mean percentage of correct responses for each type of definition was greater than the other mean percentages of correct responses in the order designated by each theory. 48 The use of the mean percentage of correct responses as a depen- dent variable represents a departure from the usual performance cri- teria employed in verbal learning studies. Since in actual practice children are seldom required to memorize definitions verbatim, this dependent variable approximates the type of response required in the classroom. However, a second dependent variable approximating the type employed in verbal learning studies was also developed. This variable is referred to as the mean percentage of exact reaponses (Appendices M, N, and O) and measures the ability of the §_to respond in a verbatim manner. As has been mentioned, §s were asked to write down, using their own language, the meaning of the stimuli before and after the learn- ing task. The‘Ss answers were classified as incorrect, partially correct, or correct and were given the scores 0, 1, or 2 respectively. Therefore, two additional dependent variables were available for each stimulus. One measuring the extent the definition was known prior to the learning task and the other measuring the correctness of the definition after the learning task. The independent definition variables are the types of defini- tions, stimulus meaningfulness, reSponse meaningfulness, kind of sub- ject matter, and length of the definition. However, each one of these definition variables has its own levels. The levels of types of defi- nitions are H-H, L-H, HAL and.LAL. Stimulus meaningfulness or response meaningfulness variable has two levels, namely H or L. Again it is to be remembered that the arithmetical definitions are classified as numerical and geometrical definitions. Each list also has definitions 49 of two different lengths (levels) i.e., definitions with short and long responses. The levels of these definition variables are repre- sented in the control, verbal and picture familiarization conditions. Inspection of the data presented in Appendices P, Q, and R, revealed that definition attainment scores in some pre-tests were not normally distributed. For example, in case of L-H pre-test (Appendix T1) scores, all the answers were incorrect and received a score of zero. Thus the lack of normality on the side of pre-test definition attainment suggested the use of non-parametric methods. First, the X2 test was used to compare the pre-test score dis- tributions of the levels of each independent variable. For example, there are four levels of types of definitions H-H, L-H, HAL and.L-L. Each type of definition has its own pre-test score distribution, and is summarized in terms of mean pre-test score and its standard devia- tion as shown in Appendices P1, 01 and R1. The result of the x2 test is used to determine whether or not these four pre-test score distri- butions are similar in their diSpersion. The available means and standard deviations are used to rank order the types of definitions. This analysis is also repeated on stimulus m, response m, subject matter types, and length variables. Second, the X2 test was also used in order to compare the levels of the definition variables with respect to their post-test score distributions. For example, after the learning trials, the §s were asked to give the meaning of the stimuli. These learned definitions had either short or long reSponses. The distribution of the post- test score of short definitions was then compared with the distribution 50 of the post-test score of long definitions using the X2 technique. Means and standard deviations of each distribution presented in Appendices P2, Q2 and R2 are used to determine the superiority of one of the length levels in post-test definition attainment. Such analysis can be used to study the differential effect of the levels of each definitional variable on definition attainment. The third aspect of this part of the study is concerned with the effect of the learning task on definition attainment by compar- ing the distributions of pre- and post-test scores. The X2 tech- nique was also used to test whether or not there was a significant difference between the pre-test scores and post-test scores. Such comparison is made using the answers of the §s who received verbal, picture, or no familiarization. Hence, for each type of familiariza- tion there is a X2 value which shows the degree of change in defini- tion attainment due to the familiarization procedure and the learning trials. In the previous analyses the pre-test or post-test scores were pooled over all the types of definitions. It was not possible, then, to study the effect of the independent definition variables on each of H-H, L-H, H-L, and L-L lists. Therefore more Specific analyses were performed in order to study the distributions of the pre-test or postatest relevant to the types of definitions under each famil- iarization treatment. The independent variables are (1) types of definitions (H-H, L-H, HAL, LmL), (2) stimulus meaningfulness (H, L), (3) response meaningfulness (H, L), (4) subject matter (numerical, geometrical 51 definition), and (5) length (short, long). But in the analysis of pre- or post-test scores of each type of definition, the variables (1) types of definition, (2) stimulus meaningfulness, and (3) response meaning- fulness are excluded: and the analysis is limited in order to study the effect of the levels of either subject matter or length variables on pre- or post-test scores of each type of definition under the famil- iarization treatments (Appendices S, T, U and V). In addition, X2 tests of both pre- and post-test scores for each type of definition under the familiarization treatments were computed in order to give an indication of degree of change in attaining these definitions due to familiarization and learning. It will be observed that the results of the first part of the design concerning the generation of definition meaningfulness are pre- sented in Appendix F. The results of the second and third part of this study related to the Se answers in the test trials and the Se defini- tional attainment will be presented in the following chapter. CHAPTER III RESULTS The presentation of the results will follow the design and statistical procedure as shown on page 45. The first portion of the results is reported in Appendix F, but a brief summary will be given below. The second and third parts are concerned with the results of Ss' responses in the actual learning task and in the pre- and post- definition attainment conditions. DEFINITIONAL MEANINGFULNESS The following discussion is centered around the scaling of definition meaningfulness (m). The distribution of stimulus mean- ingfulness was bimodal, while the definition or response meaningful- ness distributions were found to be approximately normal. The vari- ance of stimulus meaningfulness is significantly greater than the variance of either the reSponse m or definition m. Mean m of the definitions is found to be significantly higher than the mean m of the response (definien) at the .01 level, and the mean m of the stimulus (definiendum) at the .05 level. However, the mean m of the definiendum is not significantly different from the mean m of definien at the .05 level. In addition m of numerical definitions is greater than m of the geometrical definitions at the .01 level. Concerning the number of letters and meaningfulness, the results indicated that meaningfulness of neither arithmetical nor 52 53 numerical stimuli (definiendum) correlates with their number of letters. On the other hand, there is a significant correlation between stimulus (definiendum) m of geometrical terms or the scaled vocabulary m and number of letters. There is, moreover, significant correlation between the response (definien) number of words and reSponse m in case of arithmetical, numerical and geometrical items. The high positive cor- relations showed that the shorter the response the higher was its mean- ingfulness. While the intercorrelation coefficients between stimulus m, or reSponse m, and their correSponding lengths are not significantly dif- ferent from zero at the .01 level, it is found that each one of these variables correlates significantly with definition m. For example, definition m correlates significantly and positively with stimulus m. The significant correlation between m of the definitions indicated that when definition m was high response m was high, reaponse stan- dard deviation was small, and reSponse number of words was few. Investigation of the results shows that partialling any group of variables out of the correlation of definition m with other vari- ables does not change the zero order correlation coefficient. How- ever, the correlation of response m, and reSponse number of words changes significantly, when definition m or definition m plus other variables are partialled out, and dropped to a value of which is not far from zero at the .05 level._ Again, reSponse m and stimulus m have been found to have an insignificant correlation. Once the definition m or other variables beside definition m were partialled out, all the new correlations differed significantly from their zero order 54 correlation. These results emphasize the role of definition m and its relation with response m or stimulus m. For example, the new higher correlations resulted from partiallizing definition m may mean (1) when response m was high the length of the response was not necessarily high or low (2) when response m was high, the stimulus m was also high. The composite definition m showed resemblance to the actual definition m except that the former correlated with stimulus m higher than its correlation with response m at the .01 level of significance. But in case of definition m, the entire preceding statement is reversed except that the difference is not significant at the .05 level. The meaningfulness value of the composite reSponse is obtained by adding the m of the individual words (vocabulary) which compose the response. It has been noticed that the composite reSponse m correlates significantly at the .01 level, only with the standard deviation of either stimulus m or response m. However, the correlation between response m and composite reSponse m is almost zero at the .05 level. DIFFERENTIAL EFFECT OF MEANINGFULNESS OF DEFINITION COMPONENTS ON SUBJECTS' LEARNING This part will present separately the results of control, verbal and picture familiarization on the §fs test-trial scores. In present- ing each of them, Kendall rank order correlation coefficients between the actual arrangement of the types of definitions according to the mean.percentage of correct reSponses and the expected theoretical orders will be computed. In instances for which the correlation proved to be perfect, z score will be used to determine whether for each type 55 of definition the proportion of correct responses is significantly greater than the other proportions as designated in the theoretical order. A. Rank Order offthe Types of Definitions According to the Percen- tage of Correct Responses for the Control Treatment Correct reSponses have been defined (page 45) as the answers which were exactly similar to the learned responses, or the reproduc- tions which were almost similar to the exact reSponses, but stated in slightly different forms. It contains the percentages of correct responses for the short, long, numerical, geometrical and the comp bined definitions. The next to the last column in Appendix J contains the sum of the percentages of correct responses per test trial. This sum is apprOpriate because the number of responses per test trial is the same for all test trials. The last column represents the mean percentage of correct responses of the test trials, and these values will be used to determine the rank of the definition types. ' Table 2 presents the rank order of the definitions order accord- ing to the percentage of correct reSponses. The rank order of the type of definitions has been correlated with both the rank order suggested by the theory that emphasizes the role of response (H-H, L-H, H-L, LAL), and the suggested order of the theory emphasizing the role of’stimulus (H-H, HAL, L-H, L-L). The Spearman rank order correlation coefficient could have been used to test the degree of association between either of the suggested findings and the present findings but its available probability table covers only the case of perfect correlation (Siegel, 1956: P. 285). Instead, Kendall rank order correlation coefficient H23 no. 23 e. a... 3.3383... SH. -58. s3 . Se. m e m a defiance defleeaeoe Nee. .584 a3 . So. a N m a scanning defines SA . new. «so. 584 e n N a eeflaaaen use.” men. nmm. New. -53. n a N a codpgon .395 Sn . Se. mi. .084 e m N a 23353 8588 mam“ h. Mm“ t 3 a? nun gun at" mum on; we En. m m l m e 8 . e n N . a 0:81:95. H9555 on» ca. 235.509 no was on» .3.“ @0939QO 900.200 ho owapcoaom so: 23 .wo .320 E "N am. 56 57 is used and designated by']’ in Table 2. The probability of obtaining the “V value, for one tailed test is shown in columns seven and nine of the preceding table (Table 2). The .05 level has been chosen as the level of significance of’l’ . Investigation of Table 2 reveals that the theory emphasizing the m of the response is supported in two cases: First, when all the defi- nitions~of the task are considered: and second, when the long defini- tions are considered. The same theory has been rejected in three other cases, namely with short, numerical and geometrical definitions. On the other hand, the theory which emphasized the m of the stimulus position is accepted only in the case of numerical definitions and is rejected in the remaining cases. Again, while each theory suggests a different arrangement for the types of definitions, both of them assume that there must be a signifi- cant difference between any two types of definitions. For example, the theory which emphasizes the role of response m, and which suggests that the following order H-H, L-H, H-L, L-L, requires a significant differ- ence between H-H and.L-H, H-L, or LAL as well as between any of L-H, H-L and.LAL conditions. In other words, the proportion of correct responses per test trial of H-H definitions must be significantly greater than the proportion of correct responses per test trial of either L-H, HAL, or LAL definitions. The presence of significant differences insures that the discrepancies between the proportions of correct responses are not due to chance but are caused by differences in the location of m. The combined and long definitions have been shown (Table 2) to follow the sequence which is stated by the response m theory, while 58 numerical definitions follow the sequence which is stated by the stimu- lus m theory. The normal distribution table and its standard score z are used to test whether each proportion of correct responses is sig- nificantly greater than the other proportion in the order designated by each theory. The results are shown in Table 3. TABLE 3: z Values for the Differences Between Proportions of Correct Responses in the Control Treatment i-. Types of Definition L-L H-L L-H Kind of Task H-H 13.0** 12.8"".I 8.3** Combined Definitions 17.8" 12. 5" 8.0" Long Definition 10.6** 1.85* 3.3** Numerical Definition L-H 5.4** 4.0MI Combined Definitions 8.3** 4.2** Long Definition 6.7** 1.64* Numerical Definition HAL 1.46 Combined Definitions 4.2** Long Definition 8.5** Numerical Definition * Significant difference at the .05 level (one tail test) **Significant difference at the .01 level (one tail test) Table 3 reveals that there are significant differences between the types of definition, using the combined definitions, except in the case where HAL is not significantly greater than.LAL at the .05 .\° 4. 8C) 60] 40. 20, y I ‘- / . ' :‘v” f // ‘H LC ‘L4_C CODIFO| !OQ ' 2 if?) -w‘. o—---—@H°HC ’ a” ”_.L44C 8’), ,h/ 'r _.~ “.4 H'LC J /" ’2}: - a " ’ L-LC f3"; / a ’ i ’ ,2 a 24 n ‘l 4 9\CI‘T (St—fin 1 f v v 'CXL 1 2 3 4 5 .————aFFFK: 91’) -V' e / ,.- ...L°HC 6k; ,// 'aH'LL: , fl ‘1 ‘ / / 4C) a ,. x "vL'LC T r - / / ZCL // .1: . g' “(z Long 0 T4 ’4 . as r O ! 2 3 4 5 f 1".er .‘3 FA .95? Trial I" Y“- Q T to 0* O""’:. /‘ / .xe—“r‘H-H C SO ///’ 1p" “qJHC so 1" AH LC / j)? ~~ILvLC _ AK 404 1” /:./ y. ‘ Ifi// 20y z/ j Control 0 T V fl 1 “301 l 2 3 4 5 H-HC i /_q—-"’l 801 8/ r fiH-LC _’/» "0"" L’HC J .fia,’ 6O.4 l/L’I' / R I ‘ .L-LC 404 cf 1 ‘_ I" , gr 2Q a " y / Numerical i 1’ 3 4 5 ‘ I .L-HC A ./ “ 60.. / ’m/ ,‘ L'LC j .4“ ’H‘LC / / ./ 1’ . / W Tr’ "I"../ f j 1 , / ””' 2Q d, ,/ j / Gaomtrlcel 1 /. O l 2 3 4 5 Test Trial T’s Pernen’are of Jorrect Responses Per Test Trial For 60 level. However, in the case of long definitions all the differences are significant at the .01 level. It has been mentioned that the results of numerical definitions followed the theory which emphasizes the stimulus position. The dif- ferences between the proportions of correct responses in this case are significant at the .01 level except the difference between HAL and L-H which is significant at the .05 level. Figure 1 shows the corresponding curves of the preportion of correct responses per test trial. In general these curves confirm the previous statistical results which have been obtained (Table 2) from the mean proportion of correct responses over all the test trials. However, some curves are overlapping, and it is difficult to derive from them any statistical conclusion similar to those pre- sented in Table 3, other than a general knowledge of the arrangement of the types of definitions for each kind of task. B. fiagk Order of the Types of Definitions According to the Percen- tage of’Corgegt Responses for the Verbal Fgmiliarization Appendix K shows the mean percentage of correct responses of the test trials for the Se who received verbal familiarization. The order of the types of definitions (H-H, HAL, L-H, L-L) according to the percentage of correct responses, and under different tasks, short, long, numerical, geometrical, and the combined definitions are shown in Table 4. The theory which emphasizes the role of response position in learning suggests the following descending order: H-H, L-H, HAL, LAL. When the response member is familiarized, the H response will become Hosea no. one e. one eneeaoaeman. mam. mom. mNo. ooo. N m e a neaeacaeeo Heeaeeeaeeo Noo. .ooo.a no“. moo. m a e N ceaeaeamoo Heeaeesez mum. nmn. mNo. ooo. o N m a neaeaeaeeo when man. man. nNo. ooo. N m o a neaeanaeeo eaeem non. aoo.. man. man. m N. o a neeaeaoaeeo oeeanseo man h. an e peasanea neaeaaem .mmmmmmmmnummmmmmm ewe awe mug mum anus no none m! ,1. o a o w, e m1 N a eeaeeeaeeaaaeea Heenes one. 5.” meowpficflon we neg on» no.“ newcommom poghoo no omenseaem sees one mo .320 View 3 mam—<9 61 62 L and vice versa. However, it is desired that after familiarization the theoretical descending order be preserved. Hence, the original definition order must be H-L, L-L, H-H, L-H which becomes H—H, L-H, H-L and.L-L after the process of familiarization. Column 6 of Table 4 shows the Kendall rank order correlation coefficient between the hypothetical order explained above and the experimental results. All the reported correlations are zero except in case of short definitions which is .333 and numerical definitions which is .667 and are not significantly greater than zero at the .05 level. The theory which emphasizes the stimulus position may be con- firmed in the case of verbal familiarization of the response. The expected order of the types of definitions is H-L, H-H, LAL and L-H which hypothetically would become H-H, HAL, L-H, and LAL after receiving verbal reSponse familiarization. The Kendall correlations between this expected order and the actual orders are shown in column 8 of Table 4. All of the reported correlations have the value of .333 (short, long, geometrical definitions) and .667 (combined definitions) but they are not significantly greater than zero. The only exception is the case of numerical definition which correlates perfectly and is significantly greater than zero. Table 5 shows the two values for the differences between propor- tions of correct responses of the types of numerical definitions when verbal familiarization was received by the Se. Investigation of Table 5 shows that the proportion of correct reSponses of'L-L types of defi- nitions are not significantly greater than the proportion of correct 63 responses of L-H type of definitions at the .05 level. The differ- ence between the percentage of correct reSponses of'L-L is signifi- cantly greater than the percentage of correct responses of H-H type of definitions at the .05 level. The other two values reported in Table 5 show that the differences in percentages of correct responses for the other types of definitions are significant at the .01 level. TABLE 5: 2 Values for the Differences Between Proportions of Correct Responses for Numerical Definitions with Verbal Familiariza- tion Definition Type LAL H-L L-H H-H 2.123* 6.019** 4.426** L-H 1.48 10.48** HAL 9.102** * Significantly greater at the .05 level I”Significantly greater at the .01 level Figure 2 shows the corresponding curves for the verbal familiar- ization procedure. Investigation of the learning curves for the comp bined definitions, shows some irregularity in the percentage of cor- rect reSponses of the L-H type of definition. The order of the types of definitions in the first test trial according to their percentages of correct responses is as follows: H-HV, HALV, L-LV, L-HV. In the second test trial the arrangement of the percentages of correct responses is the same as the first test trial, except that the percen- tage of correct reSponses of L-HV showed superiority to the other three 7. l” “M 7. * H-HV e 1 QH-H v 801 M", 04 , a 4 //r- W 4 r W ./ ,' H x b t fl/ / / f'L‘L 'v' 50+ /" / // "/wL. L J .— 4 / ..._ _. I gV€;}(/ 4 4 [1"() 4Q //"/., ‘V Cy I’f / ‘u /’/ // n \ 4 //( N2, 201 1.17 ‘L H V 20, 4 {1 ‘v'erbai i O 'r T n 'T T O I 2 5 4 5 ‘00? 48‘” V I00 « .1ng LV 4 eo1 (3 /77 L LV sol 4 f," ,/ 4 60 j/,’ I, !' ,AL‘HV 601 '1 /// / _ _ .ar -’ l 40 [l/ ,' 4-01 4‘ * 2rd \. ‘ 20 y " 5'3““? 4 OJ» —.- i j Y Y e a 4 r. A 4 ’//‘F “\QHHV Boy 80. / ‘ 4 ,.H-Lv 60+ 601 V J/JfL-LV // L'HV 4 ‘ .--"",’/ t 401 W ._r’/" . . /, ’,/' 201 20. "/x/ , 1 «’7' Geometrical O 044 v 1 “r T O O l 2‘ 3 4 5 ‘eut Trial 195’ Trial .;L~e “: I"e Percentage of Correct ReSponses Per Test Trial For Vertal Familiarization ii, 65 types of definition. Thus the arrangement of the types of definitions according to their percentage of correct responses in the second test trial is as follows: L-HV, H-HV, HALV, LALV. Again in the third, fourth and fifth test trials the percentage of correct responses of L-HV became lower than that of the other three types of definitions. Thus the new arrangement of the types of definitions in the last three test trials is as follows: H-HV, HALV, L-LV, and.L-HV. However, while the percentage of correct responses of H-HV, HALV, and L-LV increases in every successive test trial, it is noticed that the per- centage of correct responses of L-HV decreases in the successive test trials after the second one. Looking back over the §fs test booklets it appears that the,§s reported increasingly confused answers with L-HV as there were more learning trials. So instead of having an increase in the percentage of correct responses in every successive test trial, there was a decrease as a result of the increasing con- fusion. C. ank Ogder of the Types of Definitions Agcording to the Pgrcen- tgge pf Correct Responses for the Eicpure Eamilippipption The expected order of the types of definitions, in this case, is similar to that of the previous case of verbal familiarization. Again the theory which emphasizes the role of response position anticipates the order of the types of definitions as follows: HAL. L-L, H-H, L-H, while the theory which emphasizes the role of stimu- lus position predicts this order: HAL, H-H, L-L, L-H. Both theories predict a decrease in the H reSponse (H to L) and an increase in the L response (L to H) as a result of picture familiarization. 66 Table 6 shows the Kendall rank order correlation coefficients between the actual results of picture familiarization and the theo- retical order. The results shown in Table 6 revealed that the Kendall rank order correlation coefficients of the expected and the actual orders are not significantly greater than zero. Furthermore, some show a correlation of zero or a negative correlation. Figure 3 shows the percentage of correct responses per test trial for the different types of definitions. The curves for the combined definitions, short definitions, and geometrical definitions follow the pattern H-HP, LALP, L-HP, and H-LP. Yet this pattern is not consistent with either the stimulus or response theories as has been confirmed by the findings in Table 6. D. Summapy ofpthe Resglts When the Correct Respogses are Taken as a Dependent Variable The answers of the Se in the test trials have been classified into several categories. The answer which is an exact reproduction of the reSponse, or similar to the response but stated in a slightly different manner, is defined as a correct response. The preceding three sections of the results show to what extent the two theories emphasizing either the stimulus position or response position were confirmed by using the correct responses as a dependent variable. For example, the theory which emphasizes the role of stimulus was tested five times (with the combined definitions, long, short, numerical and geometrical definitions) per familiarization condition (control, verbal, or picture). Thus it was given fifteen chances of possible confirmation. Yet it has been accepted only twice--in case Heeea no. one no see eeeeaoanmam. “No. ooo. soa. soo.u N o m a neaeaeaoeo Heeaneeaeeo non. moo. non. moo. m N o a :eaeanaeeo Heeanessz man. man. nNo. ooo. N m o a neaeaoaoeo moon mNo. ooo. non. noo.- N e m a ooaeaeaoeo eaeem mNo. ooo. non. noo.- N o n a neeaeaeaoeo oeeaeaeo 4.”me h fin .r :eaeaneo neaeeaem eeaeaneo ence nee ewe gum m-a mum anus we oeae o m N o Le, m o m N a ceaeeeaaeanwsea oneeeao one. 5m oneaudfifiofl .Ho momma one. no.“ monsonmoom pooeaoo mo emancooaom «So: one .wo pogo xsom up may 67 1 H-H P J 80* /———’ 8Q ~f/ 1.L'H P 4 o I _ P 6Q] // f , AL L 604 / ,v/ » H-LP i r 7 4Q / /:7 4;). go] 1’77 20* / 4 1 ’7 chfurc , ’7 PICTUFC Qr—fi' T W ' T '_"‘—1 O r Y T ”‘fi "'—‘T‘-'—‘—"“1 ICKI l 2 5 ‘1 5 'CXl I 2 3 4 5 "‘\!H ‘Hp '1 ’7/ l b-‘aH'Hp Bow //// :33}: 9’” .H- L P . x77 a ./. s i ,./ 7 f’ ”x, / H l P 1 /: ,é lei-47:: 60, '7. 60.4 ’/ , . ,’7 L /f "/'.1" / ,- 5 /J"‘7 < ’/i ,g x)” 4 j 4 f! 20, 4 20. 1'“ J Short J Numemcel I) v!— T— T T“ ‘ r Oik—‘v- ‘1 ““T"““'_"" ‘ —‘T‘__""—'1 LDC-J ‘ 2 3 4 5 IQQ I 2 3 s 5 ,»——-v———'“"H'HP ,1 .MH'HP * "’ 8% 6g / L-L P " ‘ ”"7 fiuw 60 ,L‘H P 6Q )./ (.7 1 , 'PL-L p ..-” '21” 40" -'/ ‘ AH-LP 40' ./,;‘/‘“1 .AH'LP in] e/ , /a/’ ‘ /.% 1 ' ../ 20., // All 20“ [I / / l /7-f Long J gi/A/ Geome+ricel o f» ’7 ' O I 2 3 4 5 O l 2 3 4 5 “ ."ria1 Test trial 1?: 'rw Percen‘nfs of Correct fies “EIQS Per Test Trial for - ‘o' .s‘ pvr' ' .: fi§%.,'. A " W 1U? 2.1 _1. 69 of control and verbal familiarization of numerical definitions. This is also true in the case of the theory which stresses the role of response meaningfulness. It has been tested fifteen times and is accepted twice--for the control treatments with the combined and the long definitions. However, in each of the confirming cases there is some overlap among the types of definitions when in fact the theory predicts no overlap. Possibly it is the nature of the dependent variable which is responsible for the failure to confirm either one of the theories with a high degree of consistency. For this reason the following analysis will be limited to the exact responses, as a dependent vari- able. This is because the two theories have been built by verbal learning psychologists who consider the dependent variable to be the exact response. For example, verbal learning psychologists assume the S} answer which is an exact reproduction of the response as their criterion measure of learning, and any answer which differs from the exact reSponse is assumed to be wrong. In this study accepting the answers which are similar to the response but stated in a slightly different manner as dependent variable violates the condition of analogy between paired-associate and definition learning experiments. To test again whether the suggested arrangement of types of defini- tion correlates significantly with the actual arrangement, the mean percentage of the exact responses is considered as the dependent vari- able. 70 E. 'nk 0 e f the T es of Definitions Accordi to the Perpppppgp. of the Expgt Regponses for the Control Treatment Appendix M shows the percentage of exact reSponses per test. The mean percentage of exact responses is shown in the last column of the appendix, while the rank order of the types of definitions under the five different task classifications (combined definitions, short, long, numerical and geometrical definitions) is shown in Table 7. Inspection of Table 7 reveals no significant correlation between the arrangement excepted according to the theory which emphasizes the m of stimulus, and the actual arrangement of the types of definitions when based on the mean percentage of exact responses. Thus it can be said that the theory which emphasizes the m of the stimulus position has not been confirmed when exact reSponses were considered. 0n the other hand, Table 7 shows that the response theory is confirmed in three cases at the .05 level, namely, with the combined definitions, short definitions and long definitions. Table 8 shows the 2 values for the differences between propor- tions of exact responses for the control treatment. Long definitions showed, according to Table 8, significant differences between the types of definitions at the .01 level. For short definitions, at .05 level, the difference is significant between L-H and 8-H and insigni- ficant between L-H and HAL types of definitions. However. other dif- ferences are significant at the .01 level. In the case of the combined definitions, the differences are significant at the .01 level with the exception of that between.L-H and HAL which is significant at the .05 level. Ho>oH no. on» we sop pcoowMfiCMflm* mmm. mmm. 50H. new. m n N H coapwswmom decompoEooo mam. mmm. mNo. ooo. o H N m eeHeHeHHeo HeeHeeasz soH. moo. Noo. .ooo.H o m N H ooHHHcHHeQ when ooH. ooo. Neo. .ooo.H o m N H eeHeHeHHeo eneem noH. ooo. Noo. .ooo.H o m N H neeHeHeHHeo oeeaoseo hum“ .r Mm“ t ceHeHnea neHoEHHm oeHeHnei eneennem Hne gum mun mum some he ocHe o o (N o awn. m, “Mir. m N H pcospooae Hopscoo esp CH meowprfimoQ mo momma onH new noncodmom Hooxm mo owencooaom coo; one mo Hoppo meet an mqpoa no. was s. gas guacamacmam. mum. mmm. mNm. ooo. N n s H coapfisaeoa Haoaaposoou mam. mmm. Nso. .ooo.H N a s m coasacacoo Hecatmssz man. man. man. mmm. : N m H compacaeom mace mmo. ooo. mmm. mmm. H m d N coapficamoa unonm no“. new. man. mmm. m N s H mcoapacaeoa eo:NQEoo Immmi \r Mm“ \r coasamom maaasapm soapamom omco mom Aug qwm mug mum Name No ezag him m m o m s m N a aoapauauaaaasae flanges 02p cw «COfiafiCfimoQ mo wonky you momcommom powxm Mo omwQCOOAom coca saw no hopho xcmm no mqm L. (inbrh “ca-{ICALH‘AIF‘H~ Ah (1.15!an E‘ C “II ‘I. HosoH no. one s. an» scaOHeHcmHm. nNo. ooo. mam. mmm.- N s m H coHaHnHeon HuoHusoaooe man. mom. mNe. ooo. m N s H =OHstHNoa HaoHuoasz nNo. ooo. mam. nmn.- : m N H :OHpHcHNon mace man. man. nNe. ooo. N m s H :OHpHcHaoo spasm man. man. mNo. ooo. N n a H acOHpHcHHon eocHnsoo film} LK mama—”Mn LK nanoum Ianoum :OHpHuom usHasHsm - H.q ewe m-H m-m amae No ecHe m m m o m 3 m1. N H :ofipenauufifldaem unsavem one nu“: «coaudcamoa mo momma you noncommom uoaxm no omeucoouom see: one mo nopuo xcam "HH mgm*.05) for the five tests (combined, short, long, numerical and geometrical definition) was not significant. The summed rank order of the types of definitions suggested this order: H-H, HAL, L-L and.L-H. Similarly, when the subjects received picture familiarization before the learning trials, the arrangements of the types of defini- tions were not in agreement with each other. Their coefficient of concordance (s = 69, w = .55. P> .05) was found to be insignificant. Yet the summed rank order of the types of definitions yielded this order: H-H, Lo-H, H-L, and L-L. 98 The data concerning the familiarization conditions led to the rejection of the hypothesis which stated that verbal or picture famil- iarization of the re5ponse member would be accompanied by either an increase or decrease of the mean percentage of the exact response per test trial. The increase would occur if the familiarized response were low m meaningfulness, while the decrease would occur if the familiarized response were high m meaningfulness, and the expected arrangement of the types of definitions according to the response theory would be HAL, LAL. H-H, and.L-H. The differences in the expected and actual arrangements might be due to different methodological considerations. In.paired-associate studies, the nonsense syllables of either the stimulus and the response are not related in meaning, but it has been shown that definition come ponents are highly correlated with definition m. Thus while the experi- mental approach was directed towards the response member, it seems that definition m influenced and affected the stimulus m.by the familiariza- tion procedure. The familiarization.process could not be limited to the response member only. Apparently it influenced the stimulus member as well either by presenting the stimulus at the time of the familiari- zation or by the nature of the relationship between definition m, definiendum.m, and definien m. But if stimulus m and reSponse m were both affected--according to this discussion--then a question arises as to why the revealed arrangements had low concordance. Probably the resPonses were not equally familiarized prior to the learning process. Some paired-asso- ciate studies have reported employing familiarization tests before the 99 learning trials to ensure that familiarization Was equally effective. For example, Epstein, Rock and Zuckerman (1960) had their subjects learn twelve short lists, each of which consisted of a nonsense syl- lable and four members. The syllables were later paired to form lists of pairs of familiarized items. Battig, Williams, andeilliams (1962) used nonsyllables for a verbal discrimination task. Both MBMP bers of some pairs became the stimulus or reSponse members of pairs for paired-associate learning. Members of other pairs were separated and paired with syllables which subjects had not seen previously. Another procedure to equalize the familiarization was reported by Schulz and Martin (196h). Their subjects spelled the stimuli, then the stimuli were recalled after every trial. A similar step to equalize the effect of familiarization.prior to the learning process was not considered in this study because it is not an ordinary pro- cedure in the actual instructional situation. A second interpretation for the absence of concordance between the arrangements of types of definitions may be due to the effect of proactive inhibition. It has been noticed that the subjects' answers to the test trials were influenced by presenting two kinds of material; the familiarization material and the learning material. ‘When the sub- jects were asked to recall the learning material, they in fact at times answered by reproducing the familiarization material. This points to the presence of mutual interference between the two sets of reSponses to the extent that the responses from the two sets were competing with one another at recall. The overt intrusions 100 of responses from the competing set of responses actually displaced the exact responses. Moreover, an investigation of the experimental situation, showed that in the familiarization situation, learning trials, and test trials, the stimulus was presented without any change. It is designated as $1. The explanation of responses which were presented for familiarization is designated as R1 while the learning material is designated as 8.2. Thus the presence of constant stimuli (31) and two different response sets (R1 and R2) can be represented tentatively as follows: S1—-> R1 51—? R2 S1—+32 Familiarization Learning Trials Test Trial The observation of the presence of interference between R1 and R2 is supported by Deese (1958) who reported that a safe generaliza- tion about paired-associate learning states that when the same stimlus items and different response items are used in two tasks, there is negative transfer. Most important is his conclusions that holding stimulus similarity between the tasks (familiarization and learning trials) constant, transfer can be varied from positive to negative by changing the responses in the two tasks (familiarization and learning trials) from being identical or very similar to being very different from each other. In this study it can be assumed that 31 i.e. the familiarization of the response was kept relatively easy and familiar. However the R2 responses were either H or L depending on the type of definition learned. For example, R2 was either H or L as a result of having one of these types of definitions, H-H, L-H, H—L, or L-L. Then the number 101 of intrusions and their positive or negative effect on the recall of R2 would be different, as a result of learning different types of defi- nitions. Besides, each reSponse set R1 or R2 was a combination of two factors--length (short and long) and subject matter (numerical and geometrical). The analysis of the test trials using the exact responses has been done for each level of these two factors plus their combina- tion. The short definitions or numerical definitions have been anaLyzed separately for each type of definition. But it is worth noticing that the kind of similarity between the short responses is completely differ- ent from the similarity between numerical responses, because the former similarity was based on length, while the latter similarity resulted from being related to the same subject matter. The elements which were responsible for the similarity of length (number of words in the response) should have been different than elements which were reSpon- sible for the similarity of subject matter (say; reSponses that deal with numbers) and thus each kind of element would have exerted differ- ent effects on the test trials. By the same token, the effect of similarity between short responses, could be different than the effect of similarity between long definitions, since the increase of length might have differential effect on the material and on the learner. Similarly, the subjects might have adopted a strategy for the similar- ity of numerical definitions which was different from their strategy in approaching similar geometrical definitions. In conclusion, it is suggested that the interaction between types of definitions and the types of similarities located among the learning 102 task R2 (combined, short, long, numerical and geometrical) is responp sible for the insignificant concordance between the arrangement of bmpes of definitions when subjects received either verbal or picture familiarization. Apparently the previous interaction was working in the same manner with verbal or picture familiarization. When the arrangements of the types of definitions, in both verbal and picture familiarization, were checked for concordance, it was found to be significant (3 = 266,'W = .532) at the .01 level and in favor of this order: H-H; LAL. HAL, and.L-H.(LAL although higher was similar to HAL). Such interaction between types of definitions and familiariza- tion was absent in the control treatment because of the absence of the familiarization task R1 and the arrangement of the types of defi- nition (H-H, L-H, HAL, L-L) was completely different. The prediction of’Lambert and Jakobvits (1960), Kanung, Lambert and Mauer (1962), and Kanungo and.Lambert (1963) that familiarization of high meaningful material would have a prohibitive effect due to either meaning decrement or the development of a word-word habit, seemed to be untenable as applied to this experiment. According to this theory the H-H type of definition must be ranked in the third position as a result of the satiation effect; but it was found to be in the first rank with respect to the other types of definitions. Similarly the HAL type of definitions was predicted to be ranked the first, while it was feund to be located in the third position. The rank order between the predicted arrangement of the types of defini- tions, and the arrangement that resulted from summing the ranks in verbal and picture familiarization was not significant. Probably 103 because this theory was based on the assumption that the familiariza- tion procedure would mean frequent repetition of R2, which was not the case in this study. The third objective of this study is to investigate the differ- ences between pre- and post-test scores due to the familiarization and learning trials. The pre- and post-test scores were analyzed on the basis that the subjects' answer would be assumed correct if he explained the meaning of the definiendum correctly without regard to the exact terminology used during the learning trials. The pre-test data have shown that subjects' previous knowledge was significantly different in case of the types of definitions, stimulus meaningfulness and.length. The subjects' mastery of the definitions seemed to be in this order: H-H, HAL, LAL and.L-H. Definitions with higher meaningfulness stimuli were better known in advance than the definitions with.lower meaning- fulness stimuli. Analysis of post-test scores showed the presence of controversial finding between m of the stimulus and m.of the response. It indicated that post learning was more effective in this order: H-H, HAL, LAL, and.L-H fer control treatment. The arrangement tends to indicate that m of the stimulus was more critical in the post learning than reSponse m. Such apparent critical effect of stimulus m was statistically sig- nificant only in case of the control treatment even though reSponse m was also a source of statistically significant variance in case of con- trol and picture familiarization. The verbal familiarization procedure supported neither stimulus m nor response m. 10h It has been mentioned that stimulus m had a greater effect on the pre-test scores. 0n the other hand, the statistical analysis of post-test scores showed that neither the m of the stimulus nor the m of the reSponse had a consistant effect over all the familiarization treatments. Probably the familiarization treatment and the learning trials had affected response m.more than stimulus m. As a result of the familiarization and learning response m.gained a differential effect over stimulus m and there was no consistant effect in case of the three familiarization treatments. The comparison between.pre-test and post-test scores revealed that there was a significant gain in definition attainment. Such observation was supported by the results of control verbal, and pic- ture familiarization treatments. The arrangement of types of defini- tions according to the magnitude of change between.pre- and post-test scores were H-H, H-L, LAL and.L-H. The consistency of this arrange- ment still suggests that stimulus m.is more critical in the gain of definition attainment. Individual comparisons between.pre- and post- test scores for each type of definition under familiarization treat- ment confirmed the previous conclusion. Concerning the length of reSponses, the results showed consis- tently in the three familiarization treatments that previous attainp ment was better with longer definitions than short ones. The post-test analysis showed post-test scores of shorter definitions to be better attained than longer ones especially in case of verbal familiarization. The individual analysis of types of definitions with the length vari- able showed a trend towards better pre- or post-attainment for longer 105 definitions when stimulus m was high and for shorter definitions when stimulus m was low. However, the subject matter showed inconsistenqy and many insignificant results. The pre- and post-test scores led to the following decisions concerning definition attainment hypotheses. First, that the increase of stimulus m will be accompanied by an increase of pre-test scores is a tenable hypothesis and that the definition of high m stimuli are better known than definitions of low stimuli. Second, the hypothesis which stated that an increase of definition attainment will fdllow also the m of the stimuli is supported. The higher the m of stimuli the higher the difference between pre- and post-test scores, while lower m stimuli are associated with.lower differences between.pre- and post- test scores. Third, the data did not support the hypothesis that the increase of stimulus m.is accompanied by an increase in the post-test scores. In summary, the general hypothesis that paired-associate learn. ing theory favors the m of reSponse was unsupported when the dependent variable was the mean.percentage of correct re5ponses. only when the mean percentage of exact responses was chosen as a dependent variable for the control treatment, was the general hypothesis supported. This supports the paired-associate theory which stresses reSponse m.in defi- nition learning. However, the paired-associate predictions concerning the presence of satiation as a result of familiarization were not con» firmed. A possible interpretation of the absence of satiation in meanp ing may be due to the type of familiarization.procedure employed. Unlike the procedures used in paired-associate studies, the present 106 procedure avoided repetitions of the responses. Instead, it explained the response only once, using familiar expressions that were somewhat different from those responses learned by the §s during the learning tasks. This fact emphasizes the difficulty in generalizing from the paired-associate model to definition learning. The molecular nature of such a model and its corresponding operational definitions for such concepts as familiarization, stimulus, response, meaningfulness, and even learning have no exact analog in definition learning. Until ver- bal learning model become broader in scope, generalization from these molecular models to more complex learning situations should be made with caution. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Archer, E. J. A Re-evaluation of the Meaningfulness of all Possible CVC Triagrams. Psychol. Monogr., 1960, 23, Whole No. 1597. Arnoult, M. D. Familiarity and Recognition of Nonsense Shapes. J. Egg. Psychol” 1956, 5;, 269-276. Bailey, J. H., Jeffrey, W. E. Response Strength and Association Value in Stimulus Predifferentation. Psychol. Rep” 1958, it, 715-721. Battig, W. F., Williams, J. M., Williams, J. G. Transfer from Verbal Discrimination to Paired-Associate Learning. J. E. Psychol., 1962, 6 , 258-268. Braun, H. N., Heymann, S. P. Meaningfulness of Material, Distribu- tion of Practice, and Serial-position Curves. J. m. Psychol., 19%. fig 1%‘1500 Brodbeck, M. Logic and Scientific Method in Research on Teaching. In N. L. Gage (Ed.), Handbook of Research 93 Teaching. Rand McNale & Compamr, Chicago: 1963. Cason, H. Association Between the Familiar and Unfamiliar. J. m. PSYChOIe. 1933' 16. 295-305. Cieutat, V. J. Differential Familiarity with Stimulus and Response i161 Paired-Associate Learning. fiercep . _M_q_. Skills, 1960, i1, 2 9-275. Cieutat, V. 3., Stockwell, F. E., Noble, C. E. The Interaction of Ability and Amount of Practice with Stimulus and Response Mean- ingfulness (m, mi) in Paired-Associate Learning. J. M: Psycho ., 1958. 5Q. 193-202. Cofer, C. N., Shevitz, R. Word Association as a Function of Word- Frequency. Amer. J. Psychol., 1952, 6 , 75-79. Davis, F. C. The Relative Reliability of Words and Nonsense Syllables. A. M. Pszche. 1930. 1 9221-23140 Deese, J. The Psylcholog 9; Learning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1958. Deese, J. From the Isolated Verbal Unit to Connected Discourse. In C. N. Cofer (ed.) Verbal Legrnigg and Verbal Behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961. 107 108 Dowling, R. M., Braun, H. W. Retention and Meaningfulness of Material. 1- 23.3% 2.832221” 1957. fl. 213-217. Epstein, W. The Effect of Stimulus and ReSponse Meaningfulness When Reaponse Availability is Enuated. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav., 1963, .2_, 242-249. Epstein, W., Rock, J ., Zuckerman, C. B. Meaning and Familiarity in Verbal Learning. Psychol. Monogr., 1960, 113;, No. 491. Gannon, D. R., Noble, C. F. Familiarization (m) as a Stimulus Factor in Paired-Associate Verbal Learning. J. m. Psych” 1961, 6_2,, lit-23. Gibson, E. J. A Systematic Application of the Concepts of Generaliza- tion and Differentation to Verbal Learning. Psychol. £91., 1940. 31. 196-229. Glaze, J. A. The Association Value of Non-Sense Syllables. J. Genet. Psychol., 1928, 15, 255-269. Goss, A. E., Nadine, C. F. Egg-Associate Legrnigg. New York: Academic Press, 1965. Goss, A. E., Nadine, C. F., gt. g. Stimlus Characteristics and Per- centage of Occurence of Response Members in Paired-Associates Learning. Psychol. Monogr., 1962, 26, Whole No. 531. Haagen, C. H. Synonymity, Vividness, Familiarity and Association Value Ratings of #00 Pairs of Common Adjectives. J. Psychol., 191490 £2: “'53-463. Harleston, B. W. Task Difficulty, Anxiety Level, and Ability Level as Factors Affecting Performance in Verbal Learning Situation. J. Psychol., 1963, 55, 165-168. Hilgard, E. R. Methods and Procedures in the Study of Learning. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook _o_f_ merimentg Psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951. Hilgard, E. R. A Perspective on the Relationship Between Learning Theory and Educational Practices. In E. R. Hilgard (Ed.), Theories 9; Learning gn__d_ Instruction. The Sisty-Third Year Book of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1961+. Hovland, C. I., Kurtz, K. H. Experimental Studies in Rate-Learning Theory: X. Pre-Learning Syllable Familiarization and the Length-Difficulty Relationship. 1. E. 231932;... 1952. 39.. 31'39e 109 Hull, C. L., The Meaningfulness of 320 Selected Nonsense Syllables. Amer. J. Psychol” 1933, 36, 730-734. Hunt, R. G. Meaningfulness and Articulation of Stimulus and ReSponse in Paired-Associate Learning and Stimulus Recall. J. Egg. Psychol., 1959’ :2. 262-2670 Kanungo, R., Lambert, W. E. Paired-Associate Learning as a Function of Stinmlus and Response Satiation. Brit. J. Psychol” 1963, 2... 135-144. "" Kanungo, R. N., Lambert, W. E., Mauer, S. M. Semantic Satiation and Paired-Associate Learning. J. E52- Psychol., 1962, 6%, 600-607. Kimble, G. A., Dufort, R. H. Meaningfulness and Isolation as Factors in Verbal Learning. J. Ego. Psychol., 1955. 52. 361-368. Kothurkar, V. K. Effect of Stimulus-Response Meaningfulness on Paired Associate Learning and Retention. J. m. Psychol., 1963, 66, 305-308. Krueger, W. C. F. The Relative Difficulty of Nonsense Syllables. J. E356. Psycho]... 19314, 1 , 145-153. L'Abate, L. Manifest Anxiety and the Learning of Syllables with Dif- ferent Associative Values. Amer. J. Psychol., 1959. ,2, 107-110. Lambert, W. E., Jakobvits, L. A. Verbal Satiation and Changes in the Intensity of Meaning. J. m. Psychol” 1960, 66, 376-383. Lepley, W. M. An Hypothesis Concerning the Generation and Use of SynorWHB. is E. PSEChOle. 1950' 3:9. 527-530e Lyon, D. 0. The Relation of Length of Material to Time Taken for Learning and the Optimum Distribution of Time. J. Ed. Psycho ., 19114, 6, 155-163. Mandler, G. Response Factors in Human Learning. Psychol. 3.31., 1954. £51. 235-2Lm. Mandler, G. Associative Frequency and Associative Prepotency as Mea- sures of Response to Nonsense Syllables. Amer. J. Psychol" 1956, 68., 662-665. "— Mandler, C., Huttenlocher, H. The Relationship Between Associative Frequency of Stimulus and Responsein Paired-Associate Learn- ing. Amer. J. Psychol” 1956, 62, 1420.428. Martin, C. J. 2.11.1.9. Role 2.1: Repetition i5 $.92. Agguisition 2: m Associations. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Wayne State University, 1963. 110 Martin, C. J., Boersma, F. J., Cox, D. L. A Classification of Associa- tive Strategies in Paired-Associate Learning. Psychon. .S_cJ., 1965. 1. 455-1156. McGeoch, J. A. The Influence of Associative Value Upon the Difficulty of Non-Sense-Syllable Lists. J. Genet. Psychol., 1930, 31, 1421-1126. Miller, G. A. LLnggage §_n_d. Communication. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1951. Miller, G. A., Selfridge, J. A. Verbal Context and the Recall of Meaningful Material. Amer. J. Psychol., 1950, 6 , 176-185. Noble, C. E. An Analysis of Meaning. Psychol. £31., 1952, 62, (+21- LL30. Noble, C. E. The Familiarity-Frequency Relationship. J. fig. W0. 195’“, “'1. 13-160 Noble, C. E. The Effect of Familiarization Upon Serial Verbal Learn- inge is Me P52Ch01e, 1955, hi, 333-3380 Noble, C. E., McNedy, D. A. The Role of Meaningfulness (m) in Paired Associate Learning. J. _E_2_c6. Psychol., 1957, 61, 16-23. Noble, C. E., Stockwell, F. E., Pryer, M. W. Meaningfulness (m) and Association Value in Paired-Associate Syllable Learning. Psychol. 322.. 1957. 2. 1441-452. Nodine, C. F. Stimulus Durations and Stimulus Characteristics in Paired-Associates Learning. J. E56. Psycho ., 1963, 66, 100-106. Postman, L. P., Phillips, L. W. The Effects of Variable Contexts on the Acquisition and Retention of Paired-Associates. Amer. J. Psychol., 1964, 21, 64-74. Reed, H. B. Repetition and Association in Learning. Pedagogicg Seminagz, 1924. 2;. 147-155- Riley, D. A., Phillips, L. W. The Effects of Syllable Familiarization on Rate Learning, Association Value, and Reminiscence. J. m. P239221” 1959. .12. 372-379. Rothkopf, E. L. Some Conjectures About Inspection Behavior in Learn- ing From Written Sentences and the Response Mode Problem in Programmed Self-Instruction. J. Pgmd. Instr., 1963, g. 31-40. Sarason, I. G. The Effect of Associative Value and Differential Moti- vating Instructions on Serial Learning. Amer. J. M” 1957. 22, 620-623. 111 Schulz, R. W., Martin, E. Aural Paired-Associate Learning: Stirmlus Familiarization, ReSponse Familiarization, and Pronunciability. J. 166. Learn. Verb. Behav., 1964, 2, 139-145. Sisson, E. D. Retroactive Inhibition: The Influence of Degree of Associative Value of Original and Interpolated Lists. J. m. LLS Choo 1938. .229 577-6800 Solomon, R. L., Postman, L. Frequency of Usage as a Determinant of Recognition Thresholds for Words. J.. m. Psych” 1952, 1+ , 195—201. ' Stoddard, G. D. An Experiment in Verbal Learning. J. Ed. M" 1929. .2_0. [452-457. Thurstone, L. L., Chave, E. J. 22.9. Measurement 9; Attitude. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press, 1929. Underwood, B. J..:, Merimental Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts, 1913'). Underwood, B. J., Richardson, J. Some Verbal Materials for the Study of Concept Formation. Psychol. Bull., 1956, j}, Sit-95. Underwood, B. J., Schulz, R. W. Meaniggfulness mVerbal Learggg. Chicago: L. B. Lippincott Co., 19 0. Underwood, B. J. The Re resentativeness of Rate Verbal Learning. In A. w. Melton, (Ed. . Categories 9;; Human Learnigg (196a) New York Academic Press. Weiss, R. L. m Rolg g Association vgue fl Egerimentgy Produced Familigrity lg Paired-Associate Learnigg. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Buffalo, 1958. Witmer, L. R. The Association Value of Three-Place Consonant Syllables. J. Genet. Psychol” 1935, 21, 337-360. Zipf, G. K. Human Behavior and H19. Principle 2.1; Least M. Cambridge, Mass: Addison-Wesley, 1911-9. APPENDIX A Arithmetic Textbooks Series Used in Gathering the Definitions 1. Buswell, Guy T., Brownell, William A., and Sauble, Irene, Arith- metig W3 Need. Ginn and Compam', Boston, 1959. Deans, Edwina, Kane, Robert B.,1McMeen, George H., and Oesterle, Robert A., Th6 Modern Mathematics Serigs. American Book Company, 1963. Morton, Robert Lee, Gray, Merle, Springstun, Elizabith, Schaff, William L., and Rosskopf, Myron F., wing Sgre g; grithmetic. Silver Burdett Company, Morristown, New Jersey, 19 . Osborn, Jesse, Riefling, Adeline, and Spitzer, Herbert F., lori Arithmetic. Webster Publishing Company, St. Louis, 19 2. 112 APPENDIX B Name_ Grade Birth Date Sex When we read or hear a new word for the first time, we might say that "this word looks familiar and easy for me to learn and to memo- rize," or "this word looks unfamiliar and difficult for me to leirn and to memorize." So there are different kinds of words. The werd might be: Ve as , my, W, m, vgm diffiflt to learn and to memorize. Not all the words are the same according to their easiness. In the following pages, there are lists of arithmetic words, and five columns. You are to tell whether the word is familiar and easy or unfamiliar and difficult for you to learn and to memorize. Each column is prepared to indicate a certain degree of difficulty in learning and memorizing. There is a column to check the very easy, easy, indifferent, difficult, or the very difficult word. Read all the words first, then read each word carefully and decide the easiness of each one. Check with the mark (X) the proper column following the word. Do not rush, but do not slow down. Be sure to read each word carefully, giving it just one mark. Example: Very Very Indif- Diffi- Diffi- Easy E‘s! ferent gult gt 1 . Rectangle 2. Cotangent 3 . Minuend 113 APPENDIX C The Instructions for Rating the Meaningfulness In arithmetic classes and textbooks, we used to hear and read certain words (sentences). These words (sentences) might look easy for you to learn and to memorize, or they might be hard for you to learn or to memorize. Also, these words (sentences) might be famil- iar to you or unfamiliar. The degree of familiarity or ease to learn each word (sentence) differs from one student to another. The booklet given to you contains a number of such words (sen- tences) that are used in arithmetic classes. You are going to read them carefully, and always ask yourself this question, "Is this word (sentence) familiar and easy for me to learn?" and "How far is this word familiar and easy?" So this first reading is to get acquainted with the words presented in the booklet and to think about your own judgement for each word (sentence). 1 There will be a second reading. In this reading you will show your own judgement for the familiarity and ease of the word (sentence) by marking the appropriate column directly in front of each word (sentence). 0n the first page there are some examples, also there are five columns. The first column is where to mark if the word (sentence) is very easy, the second is for easy, the third is for the indifferent words (sentences), the fourth is for the difficult, and the fifth 1. for the very difficult words (sentences). (The experimenter used the board to show the five columns). Suppose I found that the first word (sentence) is difficult, where 11h 115 may I put the mark (X)? (The students were encouraged to answer as a groufi]. Suppose I found that the word (sentence) is easy, where may I put the mark (X)? [The students were encouraged to answer as a groufi]. Sometimes the word (sentence) might look for me neither easy nor difficult. This word is called indifferent. ‘Where may I check for indifferent? [The students answered). Any question? Now, to sum up, you are going to indicate for each word (sen- tence) how far it looks familiar and easy for you to learn. Second, you shall read the words carefully but do not mark any of them, just decide for>yeurself. Third, you shall read each word and.you will show your own idea about the ease and familiarity of the word (sen- tence) by writing the mark (K) in the proper column. ‘We are going to work as a group. ‘When.yeu finish the first reading just raise your head and wait until all of you are asked to begin the second reading. Remember do not rush or slow down and read each word carefully. Any question? Begin. mnm.o 000.H ewe.0 one.o smm.o amm.a enn.o moo.H wn~.H mmN.o 000.0 emo.0 003.0 030.0 esm.H «50.0 mom.e afio.m mos.“ Nem.H Nod.“ can.~ mac.“ meo.a can.a «mo.fi wwN.« a 328.233 ~mm.o an~.H ooo.“ mua.a mom.“ cum.m Ann.” «mm.m mm~.a mmo.~ own.“ nmm.~ ~nn.o am~.A ~m~.H ema.m ems.“ Aa~.~ «m~.H msm.a soc.” see.fi moo.fi mma.a 5mm.“ aee.m ans.o moa.a 500.“ omn.fi 0:0.0 ewe.“ m a soenaeon oom.o cow." z one us. on.» caches» one .nuamnm A we mow.0 000.“ 2 once» vested en. snonman noozson oonon.eeae one .omnqm mm as 030.0 own.“ 2 venues: knees ca ease sebam 4 .aemeaseeaem mm :H new.o con.a o enemas cansaaoom . no one. A 1....m mm m“ :um.0 00:.“ z codedbae no soaueaemo assess“ one «asoaaeo«anaadax 0« NH mea.o was.s z canonssen. eoanaunonou ”annomae mm HA nmn.0 0::.« 2 sense: someone Beam more.aen Isa: e wswxep Mo awesome one nameduoeupnsm N 0“ 500.0 03:.“ z . sodpfieee no scaveuemo amassed one “encepoeepesm mm m mem.o nae." u easunam . mo use. A ...o.a mm m 035.0 003.“ z , seduces» upon me soapeaemo oeae>sa one «esodpdee< m: m eem.o cos.a u , noflos on» noozoon heslmadn caches huesamesd.:¢ unopsswm mm 0 Hao.0 NmN.H z seaweeds eepeedoa we compasses one “escapeodanwoasz 00 m Hms.o ONH.H a comaaon seen. eons» A toamnoane as e amm.0 000.“ z oneness mom eases one “madness: 0 n «am.o owe.“ o none. as. see: enemas A antennae no N wmuqo osoqfi 0 assuage seam < aeseam mm a am see: .02 a sodvdsdmen oaks codpficdmea SepH xsem mucosodsoo seagdsamoo as» me aesad> emosHaHmsdceez one Q NHszmm< 116 500.0 0000.0 0000.0 050.0 500.0 0000.0 2 000000900 e .00 00030200000 05 00090055 00.0.0. 33.3.0. 0 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 500.0 000.0 2 names: pompous 0003900 00000. .3055 4 ”eoneeoeveum 50 00 0000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 00203 25 mo 0.0.02.0 0300 on» He sees no one 00:39:00.0 03.5 Houses: 4 0:030:00 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 000.0 0ao0tuasz .asan 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 0.00. on. _ . . 00000.0 030 0000. 02083.00» 0. 3300209000 005 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 0.00.0.0 0.00000000 00.00:: .00 untosoae 0s 00 005.0 0000.0 550.0 000.0 005.0 005.0 0 , -. 0:000:00 seem less on» 30:: 0.00000» .00 genes: 20.0. 30.04 am 50 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 noonmne n0 eonsnaoa 0.0.00. .00 .00 0.3060 .3 03.00: 35.0000 20.0. 300000000 05 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 00N.0 000.0 000.0 2 a.» stona0 :4 ”00:0 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 00 .0 000.0 000.0 2 0023000 0.0.0.000 messes: o3» so 03000.0.er :4 00030000004 05 0 000.0 000.0 500. 0 0000.0 000.0 000.0 0 .8008 a .00 0.0000 000000055 0:0 33200050 00 0 000.0 000.0 000. 0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 - - - 0000.000000 Idea .00 08000.0er 00.0250 20.0. 000800035 05 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 3-, ‘ .-; 0.00:0: 5.025 :0 03.00.303.000 220.00 .00 “somevseonom +00 00 0000.0 000 0 000.0 000.0 00.0 000.0 0 000 e. .00 gasses < 000006000 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 -000000 mo 0.. 4 .on00 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 :00 0.00. .0: . . . 90.080200 pen» eosepseu genes: 4 2.8002000 00 00 03.0 000.0 08.0 0500.0 0000.0 000.0 2 «0:00.000: 3-000.000-0380: one 000000004 00 50 am 532 on mass 00 see: A -. .02 a 56:35.03 3 0.8000003 8 0.0300238 090.0 00300000000 83H 00:00 117 000.0 000.: 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 0000:00000 :0 000: 000000 00 0000:: one ”00:00: 00 00 000.0 000.: 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 00000000 0000: a 00 0000 0000000 000 ".0000000000 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 . 00200 000 no 000000 030 0:0 0000::00 00:0 0:00 0:9 «0:000 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 00:0: -000 0°00“0 upoonno :0 0o0poo00oo 4 "000 00 00 000.0 000.0 00m.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 00:00:00 000:0: 0 :0 00:05:: 0 :00 00000 0 0000: 00:» 000500 0 "00000500000 00 a: 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 0:000 :02:00 no 0:0:0000 000: one 00002 0: 0: 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 :00000000 00 000: 000 ”0:000000 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 z 000 000:0 000 00 00000000000 000 00 000200 0 "0:00 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 0000000 0 an 0000Ho:0 0:0Hm 0 mo :00000 0 000000 :0 0: 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 0:0 m0 00:0o:a 000:3 mpogesn 039 000000000000 0 00 000.0 000.: 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 000000 0:00 000 0>0£ 00:0 moHohflo 036 ”0000:0OQ00 0 0m 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 000: was 00000 0:0 00:00 no 009:5: 0:9 "00:050000 0: 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 2 0000 00 000:00E0p 0:0 0:0 .003::: 00:00:0 03 0000>00 00 :00:0: 000000: a 0003. "000000>00 00 00 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 0::000: 000:0 mo 00:: 0:00:000 4 0000000 00 mm 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 0 0000: 0 mo 0:00 000:000000 0:0 0:000:00002 mm :0 00 0002 mm 0002 00 2002 .oz : 20000000000 : :0000000 5 :000000000 0000 :o0p0c0000 :000 0:00 118 93.“ Nmo.H N©:.H Nwm.fi Nmm.« moo.a How.o wmfi.a Now.o mmo.a omm.o umm.o 302.0 Om NoH.m mum.a wmm.m mma.m mm:.~ 0:5.H mum.fi wwm.m mN©.H NH5.H Hum.: Hmo.: mnfi.fi Cams 5 guanoficfigoo mmm.H :om.fi mmm.fi mum.fi fiNN.H zom.fi mum.fi 00H.“ mam.“ Nam.fi com.“ mmfi.fi :HN.fi m amm.m mmm.m Hmo.m umu.m omH.N mmo.m :wm.fi a goflcfimoa omfi.a oom.o :mw.c 5mm.fi mnw.o mmm.o mom.H AHN.H o¢m.o Hmm.H :NN.H Hmo.H mmm.fi om omm.N ovumham a no macaw mnp mo mansmaoa can no Eda one "noaaawumm wfi Hm own .N mopdzum vacuum ucoo mud macaw wmonz_amaum unmah d «case mm ow o®¢.m pmmzoa o» amonman Baum mohoom Mo pom wowcwnhw cw mo ouoom oavvfls one unawvmz mm mm 03:.N chAwo vopwwcoam ad ”ommaHHm ow mm 0:3.N candy hocoawoum a cfi comcahua mgonesn mo pom « «soapsnwupmfia mm mm 00:.N vcposhpmcoo ma aopmhm :owpwhosdn w :own3.coms genes: d "*omwm :fi mm Nmm.m mpom adds Ho mucosoam map macaw mmwsm IcowpwHoh can manpowm op wCflzwuv < "Sumac mm mm omm.m Awnsd: Hwthwc hum mzoaaom wasp panes: Hmhsum: 4 "hommmoofim m :m omN.m ufiaom on» an vomOHoco cowmou wan mo opsmwos may "mssao> 0: mm omN.N mocnmov ca vohdmmms cmwvwnms oswum 0:» mo pmms no pmmm oocwpmfiv one "cvapwpwcoq mm mm o:~.m o>Hm3p com: woman Eopmhm < "Haewowvosm ma an o#m.m Mocha pfio Inpws oshp ma woummoow pcosopwpm 4 "80flN4 mm on oom.m gonad: nonpocw ca Umcwmpcoo ma hangar a «mean side 30: waaucaw mo mmooopm one ”icowmw>wa mm a: Cdoz 0 oz 8 soapficfimoa came :ofipficfigoa sopH xsam 119 mom; is; mmo.H cam.fi mum.fi Nmm.o mmw.o one.“ mom.“ Hm:.a wem.e efim.a w::.H won.e Om :Hm.« mwfi.d mmm.2 owm.m Nmm.m mmm.: mmmé mmm.: muo.m 0:5.N me.N Hmw.m mmH.N new.a 0: e 2:23:88 mmN.H wnm.fi mmm.fi moo.“ mmm.H 3m; mam.fi mum.“ mam.d m3; mmN.H mHN.H HHN.H om:.H am wS.~ mom.m SQN moo.m 0:5.m mum.m mmfi.m Nam.~ ooo.m wmm.m $3 woo.m 83 $3 coo: a cofinwmoa mnm.o wmm.a moo.o new.“ oeo.fi mHN.H NHN.H 000.0 wmm.o m:m.o dwa.fi wde.fi woo.H MNO.H Om omm.m mucovvo 90 none:: onp en wova>ev honssn pom one mo saw one "coo: m we Sm.m 533233 3:38pm o onspoaa on now: nmoumnuon 4 “*EoumOpmfim mm nu SQN anon a 3 3.3 Icouwhon woodmoos oocoumflv one "ommwomnd me me omw.m pom onp mo epnomopa honfizn one "ihpwaocwvhoo mm mm omm.m pom no>ww o CH UocHop Icoo oncoEoHo mo uonEs: one "*hpwaocwvuwo He an ome.m ommnm oco oufim oEom onp o>mn pwnp mopsmwm ownposoow oze "oocoahmcoo mm on ome.m oooam ca nowpoooa o nnwz Uonwwoowwo oouw HNOflpoeonpoE d "pcfiom Hm mo ome.N oamcowhn o :w onco nnmwh onp opfimommo oven one "*omsnopomhm NH we omn.m pom Mommas a cw nonfioncoo ohm mucosoao Haw MH "poundm mm mm e©©.N mucoemom oCflH ouo ponp movem npfls ouswflm onoam vomoao < “neweaom am we mmw.m 9323? a no pfiomuco 8: an novnson oHopHo o mo ago n< "oHopHOflEom on we mmw.m muonssc oHonz oho muonfisn oHonz can moo mo #0560AQ Una saw one "ohnwoao mm :0 own.N Eopmem Hoswoov on» ca mHouoesz onwcz on wood ouo mHonEhm pcohoMMHo coe "mpwmwm N: no own .N flog?» unmwn o mo modem pwdzowncoapoa 03» one "mmoq on No coo: .02 a 83288 25 83238 23H 3 120 Rm.” SE am; 33 £3 83 p8 8:205:23; nausea» ca no soapfimog one ”mafiaacavno mm mm Hmo.fi «mm.: m:m.« mom.m mum.o om~.m pcoguwcoo no: pun HoHHvom movam ovdmommo Ho pawn oncwm o mad>on Hopopoafiuvosv 4 "Uwoaomohe mm mm mam.e omm.m ewm.fi emo.m wom.H oma.m owvo HoHHopom on» unsouo Uopopop oncopooh o no owvo opp an weapon ousMHo udaom < “havcfiaao mH mm N¢N.H woo.: m:~.fi 3mm.m mam.“ mmo.m maoufio a no pmgosafic an» op oocohoHESOuHo onp mo canon npwcoa one "am do :w omm.o Hmo.m m:m.o :fiw.m mom.fi o:o.m soap mad Infinomov ca vow: ohm uco upon npwz vovowo Iommo unedpoohumno Houdposonpoz "muonsdz an mm mmfi.e omm.: wmfi.fl ONN.N 000.0 o:o.m «gonad: oz» and Hanson a mo poohuoo ma pony sow one mace ma onone «omocoaofico mm mm «we.o :mo.H mom.H wmm.m oe:.H ooo.m unwom no>wm o Scum pcopmavaawo ouo mandoa omen: mo Haw ondmam ocoaa vomoao 4 uoHoufio mm «m mmm.c mmm.H mmn.« omm.m mmo.H ooo.m pcwomvco GOEEoo o wcw>on mean 039 Mo songs can ca «pcflom Haw no new one "cameo um om mw:.H mmm.m mnm.a wwm.m cam.o oom.m pnwom co>aw o Scum pcopmwvwavo we pcwom nooo nonp ooomm ca mpcaoa mo pom 4 «ouonmm me me oma.fi onfi.n :wm.H mfio.m owm.o mmm.~ popomm a ma vow: m“ genes: a mason can mopaofiucfi pang Hoasmm < “psocogxm oa mm wmm.o fidm.fi mom.H wen.m mmH.H mmm.m mnoonno opoponoo op vocmwmmo ouo muonadc opons mmoooum cowpwmomaoo 4 upcosohamoot no em mmH.H nfim.m gem.” amm.m omm.fi omm.m maszaoo uca much my munoaoao mo pnoEomzohno hHhoouo :4 "hohu4 am on 3 swan on :8: am flux .02 E BannoHCfimoo a coacwmoa E soapficwmom omhe scanwcfimon EovH nnom 121 soapacdmov Hooahoeo: onoo: I z nonpficamov Hooauvoaoom mnooz a o woo.“ mofi.fi mee.o mmn.fi «no.0 “an.“ wwo.a ooo.H Mme.a He©.o emm.: wem.fi “no.3 noa.fi mac.« or: oun.m Homo.fi mom.n NoH.m mna.: wwa.d nfim.m ooo.: emm.« :34 was.“ som.fi Sm; Ham.“ wmm.m mmm.m eo:.m mom.n mwfi.m mmm.m mow.m nfim.m oma.n mmfi.m moo.H NON.e mnm.a moo.“ :«H.H nmm.H m:m.o mom.o mme.o omH.H owe.: omm.m ooo.n mou.m ome.m oom.m omn.m om:.m 03:.m oom.m om coo: a 523228 cm coo: a 5288 am a noooacaoon moon ooogo ooaqom soon was asocoacflooo on» gas» annex. oouo>na o>dpdvno neon» duo muonssn oHonz onp Ho vooogsoo ow duo ononadn Ho:0fipou on» no poonao 4 ”onowoucH . . mono and no uogofio poop. ooouooo oaoqofioo onoau u no omonoponen onp en nonahooov vaaoo 4 "once ooncoahu Houo upoaaavo pconhwnoo HHo ono oooom Houopoa nco ooon ouon3.ufisouhm 4 “schoononpoe meoumoaoflaohom ouo moooe maficaoeou onp .HoHHouom uco pcoohmcoo ouo oooom one .oAOHuopcd neon» vco oooom Honothon mo voooaaoo onswam ocoan voooao 4 «Eowum nmoumuhon Hoo napuo> o ca eocoavoum vogsoum o :H soap noshoena mo nowpopcoooum one “isoumopmfim vcoswoo onaa o Ho mowpfisonpxo on» no ouo nuanz mvnwoa oze “opcfiomnnm ousofio owupoaoow o mo anon ono nownz mucosmoo oQHH 03p mo pcfioacco :oEEoo one "txouuoe oamco no madshom non» .ohou mcapooo shops“ 03p Ho undomflno coeaoo one nonopnob ocoam o cw mndha Eon» mo Ado .oocaa on» honpo onooouopca non» onda one ”Hooho>o:ope muonas: coma :oapohomo no mo opasuon on» mcaucfie ca woo: nonpoa HoCOHnonanEoo 4 "Eowhoma4 no no no om me am om mm mm mm 0: no no om mm mm mm mm coo: cooodzfiooo .oz sooH xcom 122 The Meaningfulness Values of ReSponse Words Rank Item WOrd Meaningfulness No. Mean SQ__ 1 17 One 1.066 0.308 2 7 0f 1.091 0.340 3 53 To 1.114 0.427 4 82 Or 1.117 0.391 5 4 Two 1.132 0.497 6 124 All 1.136 0.468 7 15 As 1.149 0.475 8 27 In 1.149 0.525 9 11 Are 1.157 0.481 10 69 On 1.163 0.516 11 93 Plane 1. 168 0.455 12 1 Place 1.174 0.439 13 8 Set 1.190 0.565 14 29 Holds 1.198 0.492 15 14 Face 1.198 0.508 16 129 But 1.203 0.546 17 134 Line 1.205 0.481 18 67 Is 1.205 0.586 19 2 Earth 1.207 0.480 20 36 Not 1.207 0. 544 21 33 Same 1.208 0.498 22 30 And 1.208 0.515 23 89 Sum 1.210 0.548 24 54 Side 1.211 0.545 25 131 Line 1.212 0.502 26 96 Many 1.218 0.488 27 10 Used 1.225 0.539 28 L12 Used 1.230 0.598 29 128 Have 1.231 0.513 30 38 Zero 1.231 0.613 32 141 Base 1.239 0.557 33 3 Number 1.240 0.515 34 9 Point 1.240 0.515 35 20 South 1.242 0.516 124 Rank Item Word Meaningfulness No. Mean SD 36 73 From 1.244 0.483 37 60 Right 1.246 0.618 38 13 Empty 1.248 0.534 39 110 Only 1.252 0.557 no 127 Called 1.256 0.557 41 32 Times 1.264 0.600 42 37 Closed 1.264 0.613 43 76 That 1.268 0.639 44 98 Having 1.271 0.592 45 115 Pair 1.272 0.663 46 50 Given 1.276 0.499 47 86 There 1.283 0.608 48 126 Addition 1.291 0.600 49 92 Every 1.294 0.570 50 70 North 1.301 0.583 51 78 Center 1.303 0.585 52 130 Rate 1.304 0.695 53 28 Arithmetic 1.308 0.656 54 43 Picture 1.314 0.617 55 112 Finding 1.325 0.600 56 44 Poles 1.328 0.607 57 19 Circle 1.331 0.635 58 136 Upon 1.336 0.586 59 137 ‘Which 1.336 0.730 60 46 Upon 1.341 0.609 61 74 Hundred 1.344 0.584 62 10 How Many 1.355 0.628 63 12 Another 1.355 0.641 64 49 Between 1.358 0-652 65 5 Sentence 1036“ O. 617 66 120 Square 1.372 0.627 67 117 Subtraction 1.400 0.755 68 108 Correct 1.409 0.709 69 84 'Whose 1.427 0.683 70 71 Figure 1.430 0.702 125 Rank Item Word Meaningfulness No. Mean £31.. 71 31 HalféWay 1.438 0.726 72 55 Division 1.459 0.679 73 77 Enclosed 1.459 0.691 74 56 Opposite 1.459 0.726 75 132 Thus 1.466 0.724 76 102 Distance 1.470 0.735 77 107 Cube 1.470 0.816 78 94 Symbol 1.483 0.741 79 61 Fraction 1.521 0.772 8o 18 Region 1.544 0.796 81 97 Discount 1.551 0.788 82 51 Measured 1.553 0.798 84 140 System 1.590 0.859 85 80 Results 1.592 0.769 86 19 Degree 1.595 0.788 87 26 Opposite 1.597 0.892 88 23 Numerals 1.600 0.768 89 116 Placeholder 1.605 0.885 90 121 Empty Set 1.653 0.867 91 47 Triangle 1.659 0.909 92 68 Contained 1.664 0.902 93 62 Opposite Sides 1.678 0.815 94 21 Single Pair 1.702 0.820 95 34 Operation 1.727 0.945 96 52 Equator 1.730 0.887 97 101 Property 1.735 0.955 98 66 Factor 1.754 0.908 99 91 Process 1.754 0.911 100 113 Number Sentence 1.776 0.948 101 88 Imaginary 1.840 1.012 102 22 Digits 1.983 1.052 103 39 Indicates 2.008 1.029 104 57 Numerator 2.025 1.075 105 138 Constructed 2.103 1.086 126 Rank Item ‘Word Meaningfulness No. Megg__ SD 106 123 Parallel 2.101 1.103 107 100 Bar-Graph 2.178 1.246 108 111 Pyramid 2.226 1.339 109 125 Inverse 2.231 1.128 110 16 Intersecting 2.281 1.108 111 114 Denominator 2.304 1.428 112 122 Horizontally 2.331 1.215 113 103 Prism 2.359 1.237 114 59 Number Property 2.382 1.137 115 109 Addenda 2.395 1.226 116 81 Frequency 2.400 1.150 117 99 Reduction 2.407 1.068 118 139 Enumeration 2.419 1.228 119 105 Percentage 2.444 1.380 120 106 Ellipse 2.586 1.390 121 58 Precedes 2.595 1.161 122 72 Exponent 2.650 1.276 123 64 Distribution 2.780 1.328 124 133 Geometric 2.932 1.287 125 136 Vertex 3.009 1.221 126 83 Concentric 2.119 1.158 127 35 Predecessor 3.174 1.251 128 24 Polygon 3.175 1.321 129 85 Congruent 3.303 1.199 130 65 Histogram 3.426 1.234 131 104 Elongated 3.504 1.350 132 90 Abscissa 3.521 1.389 133 41 Cardinality 3. 52 5 1.088 134 48 Modulo 3.557 1.230 135 79 Trapezoid 3.648 1.207 136 25 Computational 3.756 1.277 137 95 Hypotenuse 3.798 1.227 138 45 Algorism 3.811 1.043 139 75 Uniqueness 3.820 1.174 140 119 Equidistant 3.826 1.22 5 141 63 Quadrilateral 3.837 1.321 APPENDIX E Methodological Information Concerning the Reliability of the Defini- tion and Vocabulary Meaningfulness Scales Information Definition Vocabulary Reliability of AD .9795 Reliability of ND .9950 Reliability of GD .9670 Reliability of Vocabulary .9865 Reliability Method Split Half Test Retest No. of Students 25 61 Mdn. Age in Mbnths 144 150 Grade 6 7 School B C Dates of Administrating 5/3/65 10/6/65 the Scale -- 10/13/65 AD - Arithmetical definitions ND - Numerical definitions GD - Geometrical definitions 127 APPENDIX F 'The Relationship Between the Variables of Definition Components It is known that conclusion about the direction of the correla- tion must be based on the direction of the scale. For example. all the m values reported in this study are presented in a descending rather than ascending order. The high items get smaller numeral values than the low m items which get higher numeral values. Such descending order of the m value has its effect on explanation of the correlation coefficient. Suppose that the correlation between m of the reSponse and the number of words of the re5ponse is +.6. Although the correlation sign is +ve, yet because of reversing one of the scales, its interpretation is; as the meaningfulness (m) of the response decreased, the sentence length increased. 1: Description of the Frgguency Distribution of m.V§lues The m values of the stimuli, reSponses, and definitions have been reported in Appendix D. Table 18 shows the frequency distribu- tions of the previous m values in case of numerical. geometrical and arithmetical items. Figure 4 represents the frequency distributions of Table 18. Parts (a) and (o). Figure 4, shows that the distribution of defini- tions and re5ponses of the numerical items are shifted toward the higher level of m, while the corresponding curves of geometrical items are shifted towards the lower level of m. The distributions of the stimuli m, as shown in part (b) of the figure. are bimodal in the case of numerical and geometrical items. 128 129 TABLE 18: The Frequency Distribution of Meaningfulness of Defini- tional Components #— m Definition Stimulus Response A— N G A N G N G 1.0 - 1.499 12 7 5 23 15 8 6 5 1 1.5 - 1.999 27 18 9 19 7 12 12 9 3 2.0 - 2.499 20 12 8 11 6 5 27 19 8 2.5 - 2.999 20 6 14 12 6 6 24 9 15 3-0 - 3-499 11 4 7 7 3 4 18 4 14 3.5 - 3.999 6 0 6 6 4 2 10 2 8 4.0 - 4.499 1 1 0 15 5 10 O O 0 4.5 - 4.999 0 0 0 4 2 2 0 O 0 Total 97 48 49 97 48 49 97 48 49 A = Arithmetical items N = Numerical items G = Geometrical items Since arithmetical items are the combination of numerical and geometrical items, the frequency of arithmetical items appears in Fig. 4, parts (a). (b) and (c) as the sum.of the frequencies of the other two types of items. Moreover, it is clear from these parts that the distribution of arithmetical items has the general shape of both the numerical and geometrical items. The distribution of m for definitions, stimuli and reaponses that correspond to arithmetic items are plotted in.part (d) of Fig. 4. are; Freq u e '10 v — N N on A A A PLJil L pt 1 A l I“. A A A 2‘; .— lUl [CD JaiLL LAAL A . 1:: 253100“. |.| sealanlooe A A I >nw$§onuoae . N — ~ . P11.41’.iii Ma: A c. . w 5355 new: nonvosao up. :5... Ease . Ii. Oshawa-e J . iieizot v r. a a Q l . .h . n . . . 84 1 .J. . _ . I l. n i no. A , re . y. >. 11\ o. u 8... 1 I 1 c , , . : g’). L... ‘ - v . 1. . 1.. ea -’ V 1. 0| J ‘AJJ L.‘ 'naa >'j.A. .‘AI- 131 It is observed that stimulus m.has a bimodal distribution. Moreover. the distribution of definition m is shifted towards the higher level of m, while the distribution of response m is shifted towards the lower level. Table 19 shows means and standard deviatipns of the definition stimulus, and response variables for the numerical. geometrical. and arithmetical items. From this table it is apparent that the mean number of letters of numerical items' stimuli is greater than the mean number of letters of geometrical items' stimuli. The mean number of words of geometrical items' response is greater than in case of numer- ical items' responses. However, the differences between the two means, using t test in both cases, are insignificant at the .05 level. Table 19 indicates also that the standard deviation of stimuli m is greater than the standard deviation of either the responses m,or definitions m. This observation is consistent for the three types of items. The F ratio test shows that there are significant differences between the variance of the stimuli from one hand and the variance of the responses or the variance of the definitions on the other hand. The level of significance is the .01 level. Table 20 contains the analysis of variance results of m. This table has been reported with reluctance since the analysis of vari- ance assumptions have not been met satisfactorily. For example, it has been mentioned that stimuli m had a bimodal distribution with a significantly high variance, while the definition or response m had almost a normal distribution with low variances. Thus the stimuli m violated the normality of the population and homogeneity of the 08.6 memo Rea :23 New.“ o$.~ meme $84. onto amino me 4868 8nd mend £86 305 33 mood moo.~ new.“ owes Bed w: doc .52 does demo 39m 886 024 some ooo.... Rec. mine Rod Ne don 882 IIIIIIII: FIIIIIIflILIIIIIIIIIL d .m :32 .a .m moon .n .m on .e .m 5o: IJIIIIIIIQ .m use: .8 mono: .02 a omopvoq .oz 8 oesoaoom monsoon 83888 2 oooom , 228589138582 use dosage 4.3852 mo omoo cw moanefihose encamom use «3pm .mfloavedweon one. no £353.:on Pagan ecu 802 “me age 132 133 variances' assumptions. However, some studies (Lindquist, 1956) men- tioned that heterogeneity in form or variance or both must be quite extreme to be of any serious consequence. Otherwise the effect upon the F distribution will probably be negligible. The results on Table 20 are then reported on the basis that stimuli m distribution is not extremely deviant. TABLE 20: Analysis of Variance of Meaningfulness Values —— L7 I Source of Variation S .S . d. f. M.S . F Definition Components 7.5693 2 3.7846 4.915 Interaction 1 . 1792 2 . 5896 . 765 Between 22.9172 5 Within 219.4455 285 .7700 Total 242.3627 290 In addition, the results reported in Table 20 are limited to the numerical and geometrical items. Statistics on the arithmeticdl items have been excluded, because of their dependence on the other two types of items, in order to meet the analysis of variance assump- tion of independence. Inspection of Table 20 reveals that types of items as well as definition components, using F test, are significant sources of vari- ance at the .01 level. The interaction of the two factors contributed no significant variance at the .05 level; moreover. the results showed 134 that the mean m of the numerical items is significantly higher than the mean m of geometrical items at the .01 level. The same test also indicated that the mean m of definitions is significantly higher than the mean m.of the reSponses at the .01 level, and the mean m.of the stimuli at the .05 level. However, the mean m of the stimuli is not significantly different from the mean m of reSponses at the .05 level. The results suggest the acceptance of the following hypotheses: 1. The variance of the stimuli m are significantly greater than the variances of either definitions or responses m. 2. The m.of geometrical items has greater variance than geomet- ric items. 3. In case of arithmetical as well as numerical items; there is a significant difference between the mean of definitions m on one side and the mean of either reaponse m or stimuli m on the other side. 4. In case of geometrical items, there is a significant differ- ence between the means of definitions 'm and responses 'm. 2: The Relationship Betweeanbrd.Leggth and Its Meaniggfulness Table 21 presents the correlation coefficients between meaning- fulness and the length of either the stimuli or the vocabulary. It might be noticed that the correlation coefficient is not significant from zero in case of the arithmetical as well as numerical items for 0‘ equals .05. However, in case of geometrical items the correlation coefficient is significantly different from zero when °< equals .01. 135 TABLE 21: The Correlation Coefficients Between Stimuli and Vocabulary. Number of Letters and Their Meaningfulness. l ——- Types of Items N r Arithmetical Items 97 .192 Numerical Items 48 -.O46 Geometrical Items 49 .467** Vocabulary 141 .656** **Significant at the .01 level The table also shows that the correlation of vocabulary length and m is not zero at the .01 level. The significant correlations which are found in case of the vocabulary and the geometrical items suggest that the shorter words were highly meaningful than the longer ones. It has been mentioned that each item is reported with two values, i.e., m and its standard deviation. ‘When the number of letters is cor- related with the standard deviation of m, in case of arithmetical. numer- ical, and geometrical items, the coefficients are as follows: -.138. -.180 and -.032. While the previous correlations are not far from zero at the .05 level, it was found that the correSponding value in case of the vocabulary (+.733) is not zero, foro( equals .01. The latter cor- relation suggests that the vocabulary which was widely dispersed with reSpect to m was longer. waever, the inconsistency of this notion in case of the other kinds of items might be due to differences in the sampling process. 136 The results of this part of the analysis made the following hypothesis acceptable: 1. There is a significant correlation between stimuli m of geo- metric items and the length of the item. 2. The correlations of the number of letters with either vocabu- lary m, or vocabulary standard deviation are significant. 2: The Relationship Between ResEOnse Length and Its Meaningfulness TABLE 22: The Correlation Coefficients Between the Response Number of Words and Response Meaningfulness Types of Items N r Arithmetical Items 97 .538** Numerical Items 48 .409** Geometrical Items 49 o595M **Significant at the .01 level An investigation of Table 22 reveals that the correlation coefficient between the reSponse number of words and response m is far from being a zero at the .01 level. These high positive corre- lations showed that the shorter the reSponse the higher was the reSponse m. The correlation is smaller in the case of numerical items than in that of geometrical items. This observation suggested testing the hypothesis that the numerical items' correlation is equal to or less than the geometrical items' correlation with¢>( equal to .05. The 137 results of a test showed that this hypothesis, although it is accepted at the .1 level, is not acceptable at the .05 level. The correlation between the standard deviation of responses and the number of words of the reSponse was computed for the three types of items. The coefficients .189, .182, and .085 were feund for arith- metical. numerical and geometrical items respectively. These coeffi- cients are not significant at the .05 level. The results of this section suggest the acceptance of the hypothp esis which states that there is a significant correlation between the number of words in the response and response m in case of the arithmet- ical, numerical and geometrical items. In addition, the hypothesis which states that there is significant correlation.between response standard deviation and response m is rejected. 4: The Relationship Between.Stimulus Vppiables gpg Reaponse Vppigbles Investigation of Table 23 reveals that each of the reported cor- relation coefficients is nearly equal to zero at the .05 level. Thus the hypothesis which states that there is significant relationship between stimulus variables and response variables is rejected. 138 TABLE 23: The Correlation Coefficients Between15timulus Variables and Response‘Variables Stimulus Variables m S. D. No. of Type of Items Letters .103 .045 -.129 Arithmetical Items 3 .090 -.007 -.107 Numerical Items .023 -.076 -.061 Geometrical Items ”3% ,2 .047 .116 -.052 Arithmetical Items 3 . >’ C‘ .070 .088 -.071 Numerical Items 0 e g m .180 .046 .070 Geometrical Items it $3 .142 .034 .017 Arithmetical Items #4 °.§ .149 .051 .032 Numerical Items 0 o z 3 .090 -.086 .060 Geometrical Items 5: The Relationship Between Definition Variables and Stimulus Variables Table 24 shows that there are significant correlations between m of the definition and m of the stimuli at the .01 level. It shows also that m of the definition correlates significantly with the stimuli‘s standard deviation for arithmetical and numerical items at the .05 level, while it is insignificant with geometrical items. Meaningfulness of the definitions does not correlate with the stimuli's number of letters at the .05 level. 139 TABLE 24: The Correlation Coefficients Between Definition Variables and Stimlus Variables Stimulus Variables m S. D. No. of Type of Items Letters . 524*“ .206* .033 Arithmetical Items :3 s .527" .304"I -.061 Numerical Items "a .509" .009 .189 Geometrical Items :> g .328" .097 -.042 Arithmetical Items 5 c5 .376M .209 -.218 Numerical Items 8 a: .281* -.054 .120 Geometrical Items * Significant correlation at the .05 level "Significant correlation at the .01 level The standard deviations of the definitions have been found to correlate significantly with m of arithmetical and numerical items' stimuli at the .01 level, and with geometrical items' stimuli at the .0 5 level. However, the standard deviation of the definition is found to correlate significantly at the .0 5 level with neither the stimli standard deviation nor the stimuli number of letters. The reported correlations indicate that when m of the defini- tions was high, the m of the stimuli was also high. They also suggest that when m of the definition was high the standard deviation of arithmetical and numerical items' stimuli were low. In addition, 140 when standard deviation of definitions increased, the m of the stimuli were found to decrease. The following hypotheses have been accepted in accordance with the preceding results: 1. For the arithmetical, numerical and geometrical items, the m of the definitions correlates significantly with m of the stimuli. 2. For the arithmetical and numerical items, the m of the defi- nitions correlates significantly with the stimuli's standard deviation. 3. For the arithmetical, numerical and geometrical items, the standard deviation of the reaponses correlates significantly with stimuli m. 6: The Relationship Between Definition Variables and Response Vgriables Investigation of Table 25 reveals that definition m.has signifi- cant correlations with response m and number of words at the .01 level. Definition m shows also significant correlation at (a) .01 level with the standard deviation of arithmetical items and (b) .05 level with the standard deviation of geometrical items. Furthermore, it shows insig- nificant correlation at the .05 level with the standard deviation of numerical items. 7 At the .01 level, it is found that definition standard deviation correlates significantly with (a) response m for the three types of items, (b) standard deviation of arithmetical responses, and (c) response number of words of arithmetical and geometrical items. It also corre- lates with the standard deviation of the geometrical reSponses at the 141 .05 level. In case of numerical items, there is no significant cor- relation between definition standard deviation and either response standard deviation or response number of words. TABLE 25: The Correlation Coefficients Between Definition Variables and Response Variables Response Variables m S. D. No. of Type of Item Words .730’Ml .306“ .715‘” Arithmetical Items 1% s .696" .222 .663" Numerical Items 3 .703" .3116:' .732“ Geometrical Items ’2 :3 .460“ .271" .390” Arithmetical Items g Q: .400" .246 .270 Numerical Items 3 m . 536“ .340* .477" Geometrical Items * Significant correlation at the .05 level MSignificant correlation at the .01 level The previous correlations might mean that when definition m was high (a) response m was also high, (b) response standard deviation was small, and (c) response number of words was few. This statement holds for the three types of items except in case of the correlation between definition m and reaponse standard deviation of arithmetical items. Moreover, when definition standard deviation was high (a) the reaponse m was low, (b) response standard deviation was also high and (c) the 142 response number of words was high. ‘While this observation is valid for the three types of items, it still has an exception, for in the case of numerical items the relationships between definition standard deviation and either reSponse standard deviations or reSponse number of words are not quite significant. The accepted hypotheses on the basis of information presented are as follows: 1. There is a significant correlation, for the three types of items, between definition m and either reSponse m or reSponse number of words. 2. In case of arithmetical and geometrical items, there is cor- relation between definition m and response standard deviation or response number of words. 3. In case of arithmetical and geometrical items, there are no correlations between definition standard deviation and response m, response standard deviation and response number of words. 4. In case of numerical definitions, there is significant cor- relation between definition standard deviation and response m. 2: The Relationship Between Definition Varigbles The correlation coefficients between definition m and definition standard deviation for arithmetical, numerical and geometrical items are .519, .450, and .568 reSpectively. All three correlations are significant at the .01 level. The null hypothesis which says that there is no significant correlation between definition m and definition 143 standard deviation is not accepted. Rather, it is suggested that the preceding correlations might mean that when the definition m was high, the definition standard deviation was low. 8: The Relationship Between DefinitionsI Stimuli, and Responses When a Part of Their Variables is Partialled Out Table 26 shows the zero order correlation coefficient of defini- tion m with another variable. Following this zero order correlation are a number of higher order correlation coefficients where some vari- ables, other than the correlated ones were partialled out. The higher order correlation coefficients have been compared with their order correlation coefficient. If the comparison revealed a significant dif- ference between the two correlations, this might be the result of’ruling out the partialled variables. Investigation of the previous table shows that partialling any group of variables out of the correlation of definition m with the other variables does not contribute significant change of the zero order cor- relation coefficient at the .05 level. Actually this result suggested seeking the result of ruling the definition m out of the stimulus and reSponse variables. Table 27 contains the results of this step, and some unique observations will be shown below. It has been mentioned that response m and response number of words correlate significantly. waever, when definition m alone, or other variables beside definition m were partialled out, all the new correla- tions dropped to a value which is not far from zero at the .05 level. In addition, there were significant differences between the new correla- tions and the zero order correlation coefficient. The levels of signi- ficance are shown in Table 27. 144 TABLE 26: Partial Correlation Coefficients Between Definition and Stimulus or Re5ponse Variables Zero Order Corr. Coef. Partialled Variables A N G Definition m and Response m .730 .696 .703 Response no. of words .586 .622 .489 Stimulus m .798 .766 .803 Stimulus m, and stimulus num- ber of letters .802 .798‘ .766 Response no. of words and stimu- lus m .716 .739 .678 Stimulus m, response no. of words, stimulus no. of letters .712 .737 .673 Definition m and No. of Response ‘Words .715 .663 .731 Response m .560 .577 .540 Stimulus m and response m .659 .658 .673 Stimulus m, response m, and no. of letters of stimulus .657 .657 .675 TABLE 26-~Continued 145 Zero Order Corr. Coef. Partialled Variables A N G Definition and Stimulus m .524 . 527 .509 Response m .661 .645 .693 Response m, and reSponse length .731 .713 .770 No. of letters of stimulus .528 .526 .484 Response m, response no. of words, and no. of letters of stimuli .724 .713 .742 Definition and No. of Letters of Stimulus .033 -.061 .189 Stimulus m -0081 “90,43 -0063 Stimulus m, response m, and no. of words in response .015 -.002 -.074 Ho>oH #0. pm cowpmaeaeoo bongo open mpfl use :owpmaohhoo powwoaoh one cesspon oesopommdp pancakesmflmes Ho>oH no. as sowpeaeaaoo nacho ones new use cowpaaeaeoo eovuomon can escapee oesouomMHv passtHsmwm s one: $0... «3... meme: no sense onsoanoa . use a msgesapm .s sowpfisdkon Ref 0:... m3: 2 messes... one .s soflesfloe Hmo.l mofi.a mmfi.l escaped Ho hopes: meassfium use a oncommom mmd.l *romd.u srwme.l unsaved mo houses msdssapm use 8 soapwsamon ramm:.1 *rmam.u snmme.1 meoppoa X. e. define uncommon .E soapfiswmom racem.c **Nfim.n *swom.l mphoz mo poses: . oncogene use E sowpficwmom 33m .1 Inns? 1.5:... e 2032.83 mmo. ooo. moa. E msAdEfipm use E uncommom *rwNH.I *rmmm.l **me.I meoupoa mo houses meadsfipm use E mstEHum .E sowufiswmon *rmma.n mwN.: *smma.n e mstEHpm use a :ofiufiswmon ova. *rumo.1 **:mo. 5 sowpwcwmoa mom. mod. wmn. memos mo homes: uncommon use a oncommom .sooo .ssz .bpwe< .mmmoo .uaoo poHHewpawm .mmooo .huon scene chow E soap Iwcflmmm wcwHHmwphwm mHHSB moaflmwem> mSHfiewpm use omcoamom cesspom ucmflofimmoou cowpmHohhoo "AN mqm<9 146 147 Again, response m and stimulus m have been found to have an insig- nificant correlation. Once the definition m or other variables beside definition m were partialled out, all the new correlations became sig- nificant at the .01 level. Moreover, the new correlations differ sig- nificantly from their zero order correlations. In the case of partialling definition m out of the zero order cor- relation coefficient of reSponse m and stimulus number of letters, the new correlations are still insignificant. Nor are there any significant differences between the zero order correlations and their higher order ones at the .05 level. It has been noticed (Table 27) that the correlations after the process of partiallization might or might not be different from the zero order coefficients. The following are the hypotheses which are accepted thus far: 1. The correlation of response m, and reSponse number of words, does change significantly when definition m or definition m plus other variables are partialled out. 2. The correlation of response m and stimulus m does change significantly when definition m or definition m with other variables are partialled out. The preceding results emphasize the role of definition m and its relation with reSponse m or stimulus m. When definition m correlates with either stimulus m.or reSponse m, the resultant correlation will not be affected by the process of partiallization of either the reaponse m or stimulus m respectively. Contrany to this, when definition m.is partialled out, the zero correlations of either stimulus m or response m 148 or both will change considerably. For example, the new higher order correlations while partiallizing definition m might mean: (1) When the reSponse m was high the length of the reSponse was not necessarily high or necessarily low. (2) When re5ponses m were high, the stimuli were also high. 2: Meanipgfulness of the Copposite Definition and Its Relation to the Other Variables TABLE 28: Means and Standard Deviation of Composite Definition m, and Definition m of Arithmetical, Numerical and Geometrical Items Type of Items Mean m S. D. Type of Item Arithmetical 5.153 1.432 Composite Definition 2.236 .748 Definition Numerical 4.711 1.370 Composite Definition 2.010 .650 Definition Geometrical 5.586 1.370 Composite Definition 2.459 .778 Definition Investigation of Table 28 shows that the means and standard devi- ations of composite definitions” m are significantly greater than their equivalents of definitions m at the .01 level. The increase of m came from adding two m values together--one for the stimuli m, and the other for the response m. Thus the maximum value of m for the composite defi- nition is ten rather than five (the maximum scaling point) for defini- tion m. Also, because of the nature of the composite definition m, its range, i.e. variation might be nine (which is greater than the range of definition m that equals four). 149 TABLE 29: The Correlation Coefficients of Each of the Composite Defini- tion m, and Definition m with Stimuli, ReSponses and Defini- tions' m Arith. Num. Geom. Type of Item m .885**$$ .901**$$ .895**$$ Composite Definition g .524 .527 .509 Definition '3 No. of :3 Letters .101 -.087 .386" Composite Definition ”1 .033 -.061 .189 Definition m .553** .513“I .466**$$ Composite Definition § .730 .696 .703 Definition 0 8‘ No. of ,3 Words .370**$$ .306**$$ .344*$$ Composite Definition .715 .663 .732 Definition 2 {3,3 m .780" .757" .764" Composite Definition :3 ”E * Significant correlation at the .05 level **Significant correlation at the .01 level $$Significant difference between this correlation and the one right below it Table 29 indicates significant correlation between m of the comp posits definition and m of either the stimuli or responses at the .01 level. While the composite definition correlates with geometrical stimuli number of letters, it also correlates with response number of words for the three types of items at the .05 level. The stimulus m correlates higher with composite definitions than in case of definition m at the .01 level. On the other hand, reSponse m correlates higher 150 with definition m than in case of composite definition. The differ- ence is significant only in the case of geometrical response at the .05 level. The composite definition m correlates high with geometrical stimuli number of letters. However, in case of definition m, it cor- relates higher with reSponse number of words than with the composite definition m at the .01 level. These results indicate that the composite definition's relation with definition components is somewhat similar to that of the actual definitions. However, there are some differences between them. For example, the correlation of composite definition m.with stimulus m is greater than its correSponding correlation with response m at the .01 level of significance. But in case of definition m, the entire pre- ceding statement is reversed except that the difference is not signi- ficant at the .05 level. The results of Table 30 will be speculated on the basis of the previous results. ‘When the composite definition m is correlated with definition m, the process of partiallization did not show a signifi- cant change. However, there is a decrease in the value of the corre- lation when r65ponse m is partialled, and an increase when stimulus m is also partialled out. If the composite definition is considered as stimulus m.plus response m, then any partiallization of either one of them will leave the other to correlate alone with the other variable. Also, if both stimulus m, and reSponse m are partialled out of a zero order correlation containing a composite definition, the result is zero. Then the increase of the correlation as a result of partialling TABLE 30: 151 nition m with Other Variables Partialled Correlation Coefficients of the Composite Defi- Zero Order Corr. Coeff. Partialled Variables Arith. Num. Geo. C. D. and Definition m .780 .757 .764 ReSponse m .661 .649 .693 Stimulus m .798 .766 .803 Response m and reSponse no. of words .732 .713 .770 Stimulus m and stimulus no. of;letters .798 .766 .802 C. D. and Re5ponse m .553 .513 .466 Definition m .038 .029 -.154 Definition m and response no. or Words -0026 .075 '0085 c. D. and Stimulus m .885 .901 .895 Definition m .894 .904 .912 Definition m and stimulus no. of letters .895 .905 .896 C. D. and ReSponse No. of Words .370 .306 .344 Definition m and reSponSe m -.429 -eu’0’+ -9474 C. D. and Stimulus No. of Letters .101 -.087 .386 Definition m and Stimuli-ifs m -0145 '0110 -00 i1 D. - Means composite definition m 152 stimulus m. However the decrease in case of partialling reSponse m, is due to the previous results which state that definition m correlates high with reSponse m.rather than with stimulus m. The partiallization of definition m caused the correlation between the composite definition m and response m to drop significantly to about zero, and the correlation between the composite definition m and stimulus m to increase an insignificant amount. The level of sig- nificance is .05. The same relation is valid in case of partialling definition m, reSponse m, or definition m and stimulus m out of the zero order correlation of the composite definition and reSponse number of words, or the correlation of the composite definition and stimulus number of letters respectively. Hence, it could be said that the composite definition m corre- lated higher with stimulus m than with response m. But the definition m correlated higher with the reSponse m than with stimulus m. Thus when definition m was partialled out the considerable change brought about the correlation of reSponse m with the composite definition m while less change caused the correlation of stimulus m with the com. posite definition m. 10: Meanipgfulness of the Composite ReSponse and Its Relation to the Other Variables The m value of the composite reSponse is obtained by adding up the m of the individual words which compose the reSponse. The reaponses used in this part are thirty sentences. Occasionally a com- parison will be held between the m of reSponse and composite response. 153 The composite response m has a mean of 1.828 and standard devia- tion of 2.338. The corresponding values for reSponse m are 2.540 for the mean and .710 for the standard deviation. Comparing the means and standard deviations of both types of reSponse, one might notice that there is no significant difference between the means, but the variances are very significantly different. The level of significance is the .05. Table 31 shows that the composite response m correlates signifi- cantly at the .1 level, only with the standard deviation of either stimuli m or response m. The other composite reSponse m correlations are not far from zero foro< equal .1. TABLE 31: The Correlation Coefficients of Definitional Variables With Composite Response m and ReSponse m Definitional Variables Composite Response ReSponse Stilmlli -e186 -0018 m Responses .075 1.000 Definitions -.154 .566*** Stimuli -.322* .022 S. D. ReSponses .337* .564*** Definition -.123 .433** Length Stimuli .026 -.246 Responses -.095 .313* * Significant at the .1 level ** Significant at the .05 level ***Significant at the .01 level 154 It has been shown in Table 30 that the definition m and the com- posite definition m correlate significantly at the .01 level. However, the results are different in the case of response m and composite response m. Their correlation is almost zero at the .05 level. The response m seems to correlate highly at the .01 level with definition m, but the composite response m correlation with definition m is not significant from zero. Furthermore, there is a significant difference between the correlations of definition m with composite reSponse m and the reSponse m at the .01 level. The two significant correlations can be explained as follows: (1) When the composite response meaningfulness was high, the standard deviation of the stimuli m.was also high. (2) The composite response m increased as the standard deviation of reSponse m was decreased. APPENDIX G Lists of Definitions Representing Each Type of Definition H-H List Percentage: Division: Face: Latitude: L-H List Predecessor: Uniqueness: Abscissa: Hypotenuse: H-L List Discount: Placeholder: Face: Circle: L-L List Cardinality: Algorism: Ellipse: Trapezoid: A given proportion in every hundred The process of finding how many times a number is contained in another number A side of a pyramid The distance north or south of the equator measured in degrees A number that precedes another number There is only one sum that is correct as a sum of any two numbers The distance measured horizontally to a point The side opposite the right angle in a triangle The rate of reduction A symbol that holds a place for a numeral in a number sentence A region of a plane enclosed by a polygon A closed plane figure all of whose points are equidistant from a given point The number property of the set A computational method used in finding the result of an operation upon a number An elongated circle A quadrilateral having a single pair of opposite sides parallel but not congruent 155 APPENDIX H Instructions and the Familiarization Materials Instructions: I shall show you one sentence which expresses the mean- ing of each word you have just seen (in the pre-test). You are going to see the word (stimulus) try to guess its meaning in one sentence. Then you will see a sentence which tells the meaning of this word. Read this sentence carefully, because you are going to write it down, exactly as it is, without any change. Again, read the word, guess its meaning in one sentence, then read the sentence carefully since you shall rewrite it again without any change in its words. (In order to make it easy for you to remember the sentence exactly, I shall explain it by using three other sentences. These sentences are just to help you understand the one sentence which will be shown to you later. Read these sentences carefully and try to understand their common meaning. But remember, you are not going to write any of these sentences again. They will just help you to under- stand the meaning of the sentence. Remember again, read the three sentences and try only to under- stand their common meaning. Second, when you see the word, guess its meaning, then read the one sentence carefully. Third, when you are asked to write down the meaning of the word, try to write exactly the same one sentence which will be shown to you, without making any change in this sentence.) () - Instructions for the §_s of the familiarization treatments 156 157 Verbal Familiarization Material: H-H List Percentage: Jack improved in his job as a salesman. The presi- dent gives him a proportion of his sales products. He takes ten dollars for every hundred dollars. Division: Jim has eighteen marbles. He wanted to know how many groups of three marbles there are in the eighteen marbles. He divided the marbles into groups of three and counted the number of groups. £333; A pyramid has many sides. One side is called a base. Other sides have other names. Latitude: Jim learned that the weather is different in differ- ent cities. The weather of the city almost depends on its distance from the equator. The distance of the city North or South of the equator is measured in degrees. L-H,List Predecessor: ‘We count upward by adding one to each number. ‘We can also count downward. In this case we get the lower number by sub- tracting one from each number. Uniqueness: The teacher asked the pupils to get the sum of two numbers. There is only one sum that is a correct result of this prob- lem. Any other sum is wrong. Abscissa: Bill drew two perpendicular lines at the middle of the page. One line is a horizontal line and the second is a vertical line. Any point has a distance away from the vertical line. Hypotenuse: Jack drew a right triangle. Two sides form the right angle. The third side is opposite in position to the right angle. 158 HsL List Discount: Jack will buy a new suit. The store will make a ten percent reduction in price. He will buy his new suit at a lower price because of the ten percent off. Placeholder: We use numbers in sentences. In some cases one might not know what the number is. Then it is possible to use a letter that takes the place of the unknown number. Eggs: Suppose you have a piece of paper. You draw a closed figure using straight lines. Then there will be a part of the paper surrounded by the lines of the figure. Qipplg: Suppose you have a piece of paper. Then you draw a closed figure using a compass. The distance from the point where the sharp end of the compass is placed, to any point on the figure will be equal. L-L List Cardinality: Jim has many pencils of different colors. The pen- cils which have the same color are called a set. To describe this set is to count the number of pencils in it. Algorism: Bill is a bright student. The teacher asked him to show his method in solving the division problem. He explained, step by step, how he found the result of the operation. Ellipse: Jim has a circle of wire. He changed the shape of the circle by stretching it. The new shape is not a circle. Trapezoid: An area has four sides. Two sides are parallel. These two sides are not equal in length. 159 ’ u ' iarization Material: 5124 I 47—4/6 I 9———>8 uwvsc’AAC/vxeéré— i __ i ‘5'+3=(2,I3',8,7)J 5+3:(.Z,I,338,7)l 5+3— 8 i 160 HW Boa—a --'5' /00——> L:— --5’7 . 0 5'+3=5’ 5+ =8 5+X=8 (|+|+|+|+|) (IHXHVHM) (5) P. \f w V (2) (4) ‘B C. APPENDICES coeseeaaeaeasee oneness . a soasceeaaeflesee fiestas . > eeosecose Hoassoo . o mm m ma 1. 1. ea m a gig es me me a- a- s as > mug em a- an mg m as m 0 Aug an a e in nu m 0H m are an a- a- we 1. s e > are mm us .1 m 0H m as 0 aim we as ea -- u- m d a m-s 3 m Q .. E S e s 3 en us in on n m ea c mum we as a u- a- CH m a mum mm s. in m m m es s mix mm .1 u- w m as w o mum caesom one: cameos can: season can: fleece m Hoosom m Hoosom a Hoosom seesceeaeaaescm cues senescence mpsospmose NH one. 5.. pounce Hoonom comm Scum mpoonbsm mo sobssz one H gnaw/Ema: 162 e.ms o.ss~ m.ac m.wm m.wm, m.om, m.on‘ use m.wm ~.esa m.em N.sm s.sn o.o~ n.e dim o.em m.amm m.om m.mm a.em e.ss o.- m-e n.mm e.mms m.we m.wm m.ao o.sw «.mm mum «gowhuogoou and {ET Lime. a see ND man as .3 ado Tenn m.wm ems v.8 v.8 56m mum m.oc ~.eom m.as o.ms m.ss e.~m n.m~ mus s.ms n.mem s.ae a.mw o.am m.aw e.me mum F ”deceasesz («wqmmv m.wm m.wm s.mm N.ca m.m o.oo are H.nm w.mma m.am e.ss e.mm n.m~ e.w 41m m.as n. mm m.os m.os s.Hn m.om m.oa mag ~.es m.wmm m.ae m.am s.~m H.ss ~.es mum "wcod mfio Too“ flow 3.3 0.3. Name m.wm Ana e.~e m.w«m e.ws s.Hs e.as o.oo e.am aim m.ae m.omm N.mm N.mm e.es m.oe ~.nm mug a.nm sinus o.em m.om H.mm n.ss s.«n mum in», .11. unseen m.was m.wflo. 0.3 new m.we Dem H. H .74 e.ss m.mmm n.mo s.em m.ee m.ss H.om ewe o.mm H.omm n.ms s.me m.se m.ms m.wm m-e s.om 8.60: o.se o.me m.am m.ms “.0: mum I ”pecan—sou w e m N H noncommom uoonuoo no m seem noncommom poohnou so a Hoses defies ance pom momsoamom pootoo mo omepcogom on: senescence 9:68.395. Homecoo can you define poem. hem noncommom poohoo mo omepsoosom H. NHszmmd 163 MWWHK Percentage of Correct Re5ponses Per Test Trial for the Verbal Familiarization 1& U) i “3% o NONM semNrmMN omam mmmfi *0: 6°.03.U§O.U‘ ..U°\. deae 5457-313: mgtgwgnm: (\3 comm: so A 2A o o 0‘) :53 mo °e R0 ONMW 0®®NBHQO Nam: meow m '54; sass-"a “see 44'; H '34-: mmaNr RfimxoflESRRN gosh-R} ocoéN :3 ma N mq NdN NHM :HHN A E"A o u a mMNm momeuaam mono dme S d°é °dd$$§6§ 4mm &'&° p 3 éNgw gmmmmmeo ammo mfio 0') o a A o (\O\OL\- mammeb-OV «Nan-0 #3503 m 3 use; a'nmsess $644 A’d‘ g mmmm o wwfimmw mwoq mgmfl o a g omnm emeNNmmo wwwm HCQN n 64$; gases A666 6&°& 3 mean $§$ wHwNfi mmwm mm 3 o A A 8 MHOCD coacxdkocomm :roxom H004? % N 6K0 fifi$6fi°0N 'Ném $NJ° e we: menu 3 men BMNg m a +3 5 in a mnew aeeqewmo mNmm mgpq O I O 0 o O O 0 O O O O O o a co mm on N H owH m HNQ HNSMN ”a w a g s - 1 0 «mm mm Amm mm 0mm w 2 :77 11T7910711177fl1177 p :mgmg mgmqqmgmgmqmgsmgmq ”5% 7 t ~° : a m E o g E o o o n o s o O o m A z u s on o.~m~ o.ms 7w.oml, m.wc A.os [wamm are ~.mm N.eAH s.mm m.mm ~.sm m.NA n.e ewe m.ws s.esm «.As 0.0m m.am o.os m.m mun N.mw m.wss o.sm o.em 0.3m o.wm ~.ec mum "HwOflhfiofiomU N.es m.omm «.mc m.om s.ms wo.mm e.w ewe n.mm s.em~ o.ms e.me m.oo 5.0m m.wm are m.me m.wam m.ws s.sn m.wm e.mm N.N~ mes A.As e.mnm o.mm N.mm A.me m.ae m.oe mam ”HGOHAQSZ m.om m.wsA s.mw, o.on mo.mm e.e e.~ awn s.mm m.are e.me m.wm N.mm m.om e.m mam N.mN H.wmA s.mm o.oe m.sA o.oH 0.: :14 e.es m.amm «.mm m.om m.wm m.aa ne.ss mum "wson m.om e.emm m.sw N.mw m.wa m.fis mo.mmi ere ~.en A.Ham N.es s.sm «.mm m.an s.mA ewe A.em m.wmm 0.0m A.As m.wm e.mm e.em mug s.mm m.er m.wm o.em o.sm o.ns o.me mum ”pAonm _ m.sw m.www, weeds! c.mm A.mw, ,flnmn mawm are m.as c.0om m.wm m.am m.me m.om e.sA are A.ms m.mAm s.ns m.wm m.wm me.sm m.mA min m.om m.oos A.Am mo.nm A.mm mm.ss o.mm mum "UOQflDEOU w! s m N A noncommom pooAAoo noncommom pcoAAoo mo m coo: mo a Hence HoaAe nmoe Aom noncommom nooAAoo mo omopnoOAom omhe soflpfiswmom sownoeonaHHsom oAsvcfim onp Aom HowAe ance Aom noncommom pooAAoo mo omopsoOAom A NHszmm< 165 166 o.m~ m.amm e.mm m.wm m.m~ w.m~ m.om ewe o.eA o.os m.am s.mA m.NA A.a o.oo ewe m.wm A.AmA A.Ns «.mm e.sm «.mA m.m man o.oe o.omm m.am s.~m s.~w m.we m.am m1: Le "Hootsoaoou me a? 2. meal an an s; .3 o.sm o.os~ s.As m.so o.mm e.An m.em ewe m.as m.aom m.am m.ws m.we s.em s.AA mug m.mm m.woA m.mo N.me m.am m.ma m.w mom .Huoaaossz .1N.© m.mm m.m m.om m.m, o.oo o.oo man e.sa A.sm s.nm m.wm A.AA o.oA m.A 41m m.om m.wmA m.wmA m.ws m.om m.AA m.A mun m.~e m.nAm o.me 0.0m m.we m.am c.~H mum "ween o.m~ n.fl~¢ e.mm1| m.wm m.om m.wn o.mm ewe e.om m.wmm o.os A.sm m.am m.ws m.wm ewe ~.mm A.ce~ m.mw s.em m.wm H.se H.0A mug o.Ao o.mom s.mm m.ws s.me 0.0m A.e~ mum tar. - in. ”steam m.ea m.ow m.~A w.em e.wA «.mA o.AH awe o.sm A.osA m.wn o.oe e.mm m.mm m.wfi ewe m.am m.osA s.ce m.wn m.om m.am m.m mug o.mm o.oe~ m.ws m.we m.oo m.ws s.mA mum xii, "Ramsey m e m m A noncommom nooxm noncommom noexfi mo m coo: Ho & Hence HowAe pmoe Aom monsomoom pcoxm mo omousoOAom omha soauflnamon psoepoer HOApsou one Aom HoHAe pooh Aom noncommom pooxm Ho omopsovom z xHDzmmmn EWWHN Percentage of Exact Responses Per Test Trial for the Verbal Familiarization 1W (a i “as. m 050““ swoon O\®L\O ON-‘JO MONO “8 65°& 6&'& seem d8°6 meme gr: Arte-43¢“ \OHSW m N NHSKC'V \OHNM ‘32 0 > m 0 m ‘H: °:. $10) H03“ [\wa \ooomoo (\mom \O\O<'\H é 684$ dads "ae °'&' J’Jé q Nl\O\O OQWH‘ $373.41 g8® J H H 43:: N NH! M NM H H H NH M HH we. 0 > ‘3 HSCDN (DO\O\N Now!“ 53:30 413““ of! eeee eeeo eeee eeee .900 $4 W NBRE "\NH H\0H H33 NOM\O [-1 \ON BMBCD Nd‘H MN“ [\Md'd' +3 to O E-' A amen emmm mend MNNW dNHM ° s ‘6d4 meme secs 465° 'JA' ‘2; RNW31 (\mmoo de' d'NNg fiNd’j 0 to £2 8. :8 WN‘nd’ N®\O\O CDWON WWBN “(DA-2'0 0 O O I O O I I O O O O O O O 0 O O O O a: m ammo mm H UN H L\ max 1 Mann ma J mwgi gwwg SHH '2 Q 'H N0\NN 035W din-1&0 MOHO O\L\L\Vl O O O 0 O 0 0 O O 0 0 0 O 0 O O 0 I 0 O O N \00\ N OM N\0(\O mom O\<"\O\ o en fin the-03$ N HO vie-4mg R to 3 5 o mmmw NHMN mNNo homo MdON h coco eoeo eeee eeec eeee o HI rims-Mn 0\OO\O \oe-umo anNo HNOO p. m Hal-1H o N :1- CH“ g; g mmng mmnn mm q"mmqqgmmq or! llll llll uu’Te'deaaufiuan'l' a mqmg mnmg mnmggmnmqgmqmq 4.) co ‘4 a a : :9 3 o o n o 8 Q o 0‘) 1—1 C! MMWHO Percentage of Exact Responses Per Test Trial fer the Picture Familiarization 1% m o It) :5 fig NNNN :owo ommN HQm: mmmq 00.0 .00. 0.0 .0. 0 Now H no mm gm m meH 5m wNN n :0 $3 N3N NNH: £3 3 U) cHa) ca Ra sass “c9“ QNss wwwN ssss MHHH o ONON mm- H O\ 1§ Hm: ngg BBO? MNfid m m3 fig MHHH MHNN N NHNH m N 90 3 q NQnH mmmm wowfl omaw NQNM O... 9... 000° 0... mum H momH N NM 6: E W has $R0N FMNN m we maN «fl .p 0) 59. a dHNQ mwow OMdm Hon 00mm 00°. 00.0 0.00 o d mon ammN mm Hi Hm: m fig: msnfi WNHfl mi; OMH m 0 U) S % BGON anw mmmm Hoow Hwow 00.0 000 0000 0.9. o 0mm m m Hoom NBQ m n wNN BR? win mNMN QN 3 .p o 5 m MQQQ HdNH QQQQ mfiqw Qmmm O .00 O o O o N mmn m mm ammo 0 HH mmmn o WHNg \ofizrw :r o (“$de {\N m w :3 s ° A 2 H “ass 9%?% mono see. ness 0 H m 0 NW oomo H NH omw m mamm mmHN No C HmH fiRo N o E g mmqg mmqq mmqg"mm ggmmgq H Ulll! llll outlinnfianunu p gmqmq mqmq mgmggzgmgsmgmg E H fi ~ 3 2 M g g g g o o o o o a o m g z w on. mm. «o. mm. mm. sow macs ofim. mfi.mm am. HH. Ho. mo. om. new pgogm npmqoq Ha. mH. Ho. 5H. mm. sow .ooo mmo.n mn.H $3. NN. No. mH° cw. dwN cadz Magus: poonnsm oz. mH. Ho. 5H. mm. mum A 530.- oa.H es. mm. No. ma. ow. mmm m a omcoamom mo. Ho. 00. Ho. mo. mom 4 33.: EA: an. 0:. no. mm. 8. 03 m s 3953 NH. do. oo. Ho. mm. ems Aug Hm. 3m. «0. Hmo no. 03H A1: 00. oo. oo. oo. co.H on“ mag amm.n No.5NH mm. we. so. 0:. on. mHH mum «soapficflmoo mo «came a Nx .>oa .pm can: N \H o COHHsQanmHQ onoom amoenmnm z Hm>mq oHanu¢> sOHpHsHmoQ psmspmoue Houpzoo mo ammo CH moHDaHpm> GOHHHCHmoQ nHHz J gamma: menoom Humaneum no COHpanupmHQ Hd20HHnomoum 169 on. Hu.H ow. co. sH. m N no. m:.H on. mm. oH. mm H HONo mw.:N aw. wo.H 0:. mN. Hm. aN: o whoom pmoetopm mm. mo.H mm. Hm. on. sou msoq amo.- m~.m mm. 3N.H we. mm. mm“ sow phonm spmcoq mm. HH.H ms. am. “no sow oooo mno.u mm.n ow. HN.H ma. Hm. 3N» :mN oE52 poupqz voonnsm om. mo.H mm. emu wN. mew A amo.- mo.:H mm. am.H Hm. mme mm. mam m s uncommom am. mo.H mm. on. mm. mom 4 omHoa mm.NH ow. mN.H Hm. mm. Nm. owm m a msHseflpm Hm. mo.H mm. mm. ow. mmH Hug me. HH.H an. on. em. 03H gum ow. oo.H um. em. mm. and mag mmH.- mm.Hs nu. mm.H mo. 0H. 0H. mad mum macapficamoo mo masks a Nx .>oa .pm cue: N H o soapsnfinpmfia ouoom pmosupmom z H¢>oq casuan¢> sofipflcfimon pzmspaoue Hoppsoo mo ommu :H moHnaHua> sOHpHsHmcn nqu moaoow pmoelpmom Ho COHanHuHmHQ HdcowahomOhm mm xHozmmm< 170 mm. 0N. co. 3H. ow. omN macs Nmo. om.o Na. NH. so. No. mm. omN spasm gumqoq omw HN. so. NHO mm. CNN 008 woo.- NH.H mm. HN. mo. oH. am. omN .352 house: poonnsm mm. nN. no. nH. om. con H . muo. mm.m as. NH. no. No. mm. omN m a ougomuom 0N. Ho. Ho. Ho. mm. owN H mom.» Nm.mo No. mm. mo. ON. NN. ONm m s mngaHHm Nm. mo. Ne. Ho. Na. wNH HTH am. Nm. mo. mm. mm. :NH 41m oo. oo. oo. oo. oo.H 30H mug HNH.- mm.m:H 0N. Na. NH. NH. 0N. mHH mum mquHHcHHoa Ho momma u NN .pca .»m can: N Hwy o :OHHanupmHa ouoom Hmoauoum z Hu>oH oHnaHnu> :oHpHcHHoa >|lul :oHaaaHgaHHHaaH Hunno> Ho amao pH mngqua> :oHHHcHHoo nHHz.mouoom Hmoeuoum Ho coHpanHuHmHo Hu:0HpuoaouN Ho xHozmmm< 171 HN. HN.H ON. OO. OH. ON N 8. 93 mm. mm. OO. 8 H NOH. NN.On ON. OH.H ma. HN. mm. mm: O anoom Omoauoam no. OO.H OO. NN. mm. OON Ncoq mNH.- OO.OH NO. ON.H um. OH. ON. OON spasm newcoq ON. OH.H OO. NN. Om. OON .ooO HOO.- Na.m ON. ON.H Nm. ON. NN. OON .asz Nous“: Hoonnam ON. ON.H ms. NN. ON. OON H mno. ON.H mm. OH.H NO. ON. mm. OON m a uncommom NO. HN.H Om. ON. ON. OON H m8. HHO.H NO. mH.H 3. N. Nm. ONm m a 333 NN. mN.H Nu. ON. ON. ONH HTH mm. OH.H «O. ON. ON. ONH me ON. HH.H 0:. ON. mm. OOH muH HNO. Om.m NO. OH.H Os. HN. ON. nHH mam unoHpHcHNon Ho mommy .H NN .25 .5 O: N i H... O OOHNOOHNNOHO ouoom Hmoauumom z Ho>aH oHOaHna>.:OHHH:HNoO OOHNONHNOHHHaam Hanuo> Ho omuo :H moHanum>.OOHHHcHHcO OOH: menoom smoenpmom Ho OOHOODHNpmHm HN:OHHNOOONN NO NHOszNO 172 Om . mm . OO . NN. NN. in 33 RH . OH .OH 3. NH. 8. HH. mm. in 28% .333 mm . 0N. mo. NH . NN. in 60$ moo. Hm. an . mN. no. m H . om. in .52 .834: poonndm Ns. 0N. no. 3. mm. omN .H mmof NN.“ mm... on. No. NH. 0N. mam m a uncommon m N. 3. Ho. No. No. mmm .H mnd... NH .NaH 3. Hm . mo . mm. mm . NaN m a 33pm Nm. mo. mo. +3. 3. 3H .74 3m . mm. no. mm. 8 . :NH Aim 8. co. co . oo. 00 .H owH and NN.: HN .moH HN. S. :H . on . Hm . 3H mum 303393 .Ho was h NM $00 5m 502 N H o soflHfiHhamHn 0.30m ance-cum z Hosea oHnuth> QOHHHHHHHOQ 5332.32; 8:33 .Ho 38 SH 3335 zofiflflon £3: 8.88 $4.8m NO 833233 aofluafim Hm OHEEEO 173 174 on. :m.« Na. 00. m0. Hm N am. mm.H mm. mm. mo. HoH H NQH. oH.H: am. mm.H mm. wH. aw. mm: o ouoom pmoauoum 3. R . H mm . om. Hm . in was one? mo.~ R. 34 am. 8. 8. in team 59:5 mm. mm.H mm. 0H. MN. de .000 NNo.u NN.n 0N. N:.H so. mH. NH. :Hn .352 hoped: poonnsm Nm. Nm.H mm. NN. MN. OQN A moH.n Na.m NN. m:.H we. NH. NH. mén m B uncommom No. od.H Ho. NH. NN. 0mm A mNo.u n:.m NN. ¢:.H Ho. NN. NH. NmN m .E maadsHpm Nw. OM.H ow. 0H. HN. omH AIA mm. MN.H m3. NN. mN. aNH 41m mm. 0:.H NW. 0H. NN. omH min on.I m¢.mH mo. ow.H HN. wH. HH. wwH mam mGOHpHGHHOQ Ho momma .— NH $8 :6 5o: N J. o 533$...an 28m 0.89-38 2 H33 32.3; 83383 5332.33; 8393 mo 9.26 :H mchaflé 83228 .3: «88m 9.39-38 mo 83.5.33 153893 Nm figmfi 2.. mm. 8. 3. R. é 33 men. 3.8 No. R. 8. N. 2.. am £28 £33 HN. no. NH. NN. om. aw .ooo ooo. NNH. NN. mm. :H. mm. Hm. :0 .552 .2 .nam 09:30Hm Hm. No. HN. HN. mm. mm Ncoq 3N. No.3 3. HN. 8. i. mm. mm 0.8.3 .393 “W. 30 mo. MN. 80 a 000w 0No.l no.m “N. m:. 0H. NH. NN. mm .852 «z .Qam Hdnho> on. R. 8. 8. Nm. mm 33 com . 36m 3. NH. 8. NH. 8. mm .28... .3qu R. #30 “C. an. N00 mm 000.0 03.. i; 8. mm. no. 3. an. mm .52 .z 5:... H9350 u mm? :3 :6 5o: N .HW o COHvanApmHQ ouoom pmoaloum z Hp>oq oHnuHAa> :oaawanuHHHsdm mucoapaoue cOpruw £333..” 298:8 3?: 333.2; 183238 5H2 mum No 3.83 issuer” mo 833233 infiuomzm Hm x522“: 175 NH. OO. OO. mN N 1mm. Om. NO. HO. mm H N. .OH N . OH. OH. N O Hume-oam mm no. NO.H NN. NH. OH. NO \mmmH ONO. NN. NN. mm.H mm» NH. NH. mm Huogm camcoq mo. NO.H Om. NO. NH. Ow .ooo NNH. N0.0H NO. Hn.H NO. NN. HH. so .asz .2 .nsm oNOHOHN OH. OO. HO. NH N NH. NO. OO. ON H NON. NO.NN NN. OH. NN. Hm O Omoe.opm Om. nn.H mm. ON. HN. mm NumH NnH. ONN.N NN. NO.H ma. MH. Om. mm Ononm Opmcaq NN. mm.H OO. NH. NN. .ooO NmH. Nm.N mm. moH mm. NN. mm. mm .52 .2 .85 33> Ow. OO. ON. m N ON. ON. OO. O: H NON. OH.mH mm. NO. NH. mm\ O Huaen¢um Nw. NO.H 3N. HN. wO. mm mmoH NHN. NN.N NN. 1mm.H mm. NH. ON. mm Ogogm numcoH ON. RH 1NN. OH. NH. fl 5% mmO. NH.m NN. Om.H OO. NN. NH. mm .asz .2 .25m Houpcoo p NH .>on .Om cums N H O COHagflhvwg 200m Pwofilfimonm z H0>01H QHQHHAQNH CowvaHMMflHflfidh 3:053er noavusd -NNHHHENN HcouommHO Nouns moHnaHga> HN:OHHHOHHOO OOH: mum mo monoom Once-pmom Ho :OHOODHOHWHO HNOOHONONONm NNNNENQE 176 OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H ON 2202 OOO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H ON ONOOO 2pwco2 OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H ON .ooO OOO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H ON .222 .2 .OOO onspoHN OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H NO mcoH OOO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H NO ONOOO Opmcog OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H NO .ooO OOO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H NO .222 .2 .OOO Hanuo> OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO N202 OOO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO ONOOO OONcoH OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO .ocO OOO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO .252 .2 .OOO HouHcoo a N2 .OOO .pm 2.92 N HI O cOHHOOHNHmHO ogoom OO.H-ouN z Ho>oH oHOtuu> :OHO.NHN.HHH2.2 unsuspwohe cowpusd -NNHHHENN HcouommHO have: moHO¢H2w> OOHHHcHHOO 22H: 2-2 Ho mogoom Hmoenonm Ho OOHHOOHHHOHO HN20HHuomogm H .H. xHszmnE 177 OO. OO. OO. OOO N OO. OO. OO. OOO H NO. OH. NN. OOH O Omoeuoum HO. OH. ON. ON mmoH NO. Ofi N? ON whim .3952 NO. OH. OO. om .ooo mHN.u mn.m MN. mm.H NN. MH. 3H. om .Efiz .2 .95m 0h5¢0Hm OO. OO. OO. OOO N OO. OO. mm. OOO H OOO. OO. \OO. .lmmw . an O pmoanoum ON. ON. On. ON. OH. NO 02 ONH.- mmum OO. NN.H mm. Nmm. NN. NO ONOOO OHOcoq .| NO. OO.H O. HN. NO. NO .ooc OOO.- NO. ON. OH.H NO. OH. NO. NO .2:2 .2 .OOO HOngo> OO. OO. OO. OOO N OO. OO. OO. OOO H OOO. OO. NO. ON. NO. OOH O Hmoeuopm NN. MN. Om. OH. O2. OO oH OOO.- OO.O OO. NO.H Om. NO. ON. OO Ogmmmw spmcoq .wa. NN. On. ON. Ow. OO .oOO ONO.- OOH. OO. OO.H ON. ON. ON. OO .252 .2 .OOO Houpcou a N2 .>oO .OO mmmm N H O :OHHOOHuHmHO oncom Omoenpmom z Ha>o2 OHONHNO> :OHOONHNOHHHEOO mwccspuohs :odeNH .uuHHHEO 2220.33 .895 $332; 83238 .32 21H .3 8.88 0.22.38 .3 OOHOBHSmHH. HEOHOSNOE Ne 2HO2ONN< 178 NO . .3. NO. 3. ON . NO O23 OOH . NON Om . ON. n O. OH . NN. NO £82m 2.352 NO. ON. NO. NO. OO. NO .25 m 3 . Nm. H NW . an . no. 3N . HN . NO .282 .2 .nHHm ohH—HHOHHHH HO. HO. OO. 3. 3. NO 33 Hm H . No .m NO . N3. 3 . MN . 8 . NO chasm :pmcoq an 0 *3. “C 0 3n 0 H0 0 NW 0 OOO ONH.- HN.N NO. OO. HH. NO. NO . NO .52 .2 .225 H3O; om . ‘3. 8. +3. on . oN mcoq ONH . NO.NH NO. ON. NO. NH . NN . ON .225 533 S.-. R. . OO. R. NO. ON .08 ONO: NO.N Om . ON. NO. ON . NO . ON .52 .2 .OOO H828 .H NH :3 .OO 332 N H O 533238 28m $02-2N 2 H252 .332; 53.3532; mannaadga 5332335 OcaohHa .SHEO 33.2; 5353 HO .HO 8.88 $3.63 .HO 533233 HEOHfiONouN HO NHOZONNO 179 NO. OO. OO. O N OH. OH. NO. mm H mmmw NO.OH Nm. NH. ON. OO O Hmoeuoum ON. HMNH. mm. mm. mH. NO mmoq OOO. HO.N N . OH.H NW. N. Hm. NO Huogm Opmcoq mm. OO.H . Nm. Hm. NO .oou OON.- mm.O OO. OO.H OO. HN. OH. NO .552 .2 .OOO oNOHOHm NO. OO. HO. OH N OH. OH. OO. O: H mmN. Om.NN ON. OH° mm“. mmr. O Hmoeuoum ON. NHNH OO. mm. HN. NO mmoq ONO. NO.HH mm. NH.H OO. OH. NO. NO Hummm. OHNOOH OO. Om» N. On. NO. NO .OOO OOm.- N0.0H NO. NO.H OO. OH. HN. NO .252 .2 .OOO Hanum> HO. OO. OO. N N HH . OH . OO . 3 H ONH. OMHO . NN. HN. am O HmoN:¢nm mN . OO. H mu. Mm. ON . ON a mmm»- NO.N OO. NH.H ON» ON. ON. ON Hhosm npwnoq HQ. g 0 CM. .3“ o W 0 ON 0 00.0 OON.- N0.0 ON. NN.H OO. mm. NH. ON .552 .2 .OOO Hoppcoo a Na .>oa .Om Odes N H O Gadpgflhpma whoom amoelamom z Ho>oA OHD¢HHQ> Co mpaflfihdflfifiah mucoapuaa OOHHONHNOHHHEON HcopONHHO Nouns OOHnuHua> OOHHHOHNOQ Own mo mouoom Hmoa.pmom No :OHHOOHNHOHO HOOOHOAOQONN NO xHOszNN 180 OH. NO. OO. NO. NN. ON Ozog ONH.- HN.O NO. OH. NO. NO. ON. ON OAOOO Onwzoq NO. NO. NO. ON. ON. ON .ooO OOO. OO. NO. NO. NO. ON. ON. ON .auz .2 .OOO onsOOHN OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO NOON HOH.- HN.O OO. HH. OO. NO. NN. OO ONOOO OOOOoH OO. HH. no. No. nN. OO .25 HOH. HN.O OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO .agz .2 .OOO HOONOO NH. HO. OO. HO. NN. OO Ocoq OOO. OO. NH. HO. OO. HO. NN. OO OOOOO OOOOoH NH. NO. OO. NO. NN. OO .OOO NNH. NO.N OO. OO. OO. OO. OO.H OO .asz .2..O:O HONOOOO N Illa O Gadfigfifimg zoom amoelgm z Hobbfl 03ddhd> Gowvduflhdfiqfidh mucoSpOOHH SHOSHQHHEON Okayama .816 333.2; 83238 21H .Ho 3.33 Ommauoé .Ho OOHOBNSOHO HSOENOHHBN H> NHOzONNO 181 OO. OO. OO. O N mm. MO. OO. O H HOH. OmwO . H. HN. OOH O Once-oum NO. WH.H OO. MN. Hm. ON flog NNN.- O0.0H Om. OOOH mm“. OH. NH. ON ONOOO OOOOOO OO. HO.H OO. HN. NH. .ON .Nmm ONO. mm. Ow. Nmé ow. NH. MN. Q. .552 .2 .QHHm. 233m NO. OO° HO. O N HO. OO. OO. N H NNO. NH.O NO. pmw .mm. ONH O Onoeuoum OO. ON. NO. ON. NO. OO Omoq OOO.- .ON . NO.H NN. OH. OH. mm ONOOO OOOcoq MO. OH.H NO. ON. OO. OO .ooO NHH.- ON.N OO. OO.H NO. NH. ON. OO .252 .2 .OOO Hanuo> OO. OO. OO. O N HO. OO. HO. N H OHO.- OH.H Om. OO. NN. OmH O Ouoa-ogm 0N. . cm. 0.“ . wq m: . mm :04 ONO..- mO.NH ON. Nm.H OO. Hm. k3. OO 226 533 MO. OO.H NM. NM. Hm. OO 63 ooo.: Hm . ow. Noé mm. NM . wN. mm .852 .2 .95 Honpcoo 2 N2 .>OO .OO 232 N H O COHuBflhflma choom nmmfilvwom Z Hw>oq afifldfih¢> SOHHdNthHHHEdm 3:92?er OOHOONHONHHHEOO OOOOONNHO Nouns OOHOOHNO> OOHOHOHOOO qu No mouoom Omoeupmom No OOHOOOHNOOHO HOOOHONONONN N> NHOOONNO 182 APPENDIX W The Letter Sent to the Schools That Participated in This Study December 23, 1965 Dear : The purpose of this letter is to express my appreciation for the fine cooperation that I received from you and your staff while conducting my doctoral dissertation research at your school. Without such coop- eration, such a study would not have been possible. I would like to be in a position to discuss my findings with.you and your staff. However, it is necessary for me to return at this time to the United Arab Republic. My major professor, Dr. Clessen Martin, has kindly informed me of his willingness to discuss the study with any interested persons. If there is any interest in this research, Dr. Martin may be contacted at Michigan.State University. May I again thank you, your staff, and the students for their coop- eration. Sincerely, Salah A. Hotar 31 Red Cedar Elementary School, East Lansing Baily Elementary School, East Lansing Dwight Rich Junior High School, East Lansing Waverly Junior High School, Lansing - Mason Junior High School, Mason - Holt Junior High School, Holt - Springfield Junior High School, Battle Creek QWFJUOUIP l 183