AN ANALYSES OF FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH CHANGES IN SCHOLASTI‘C PERFORMANCE PATTERNS fine]: {or ”H: Degree of DH. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Rodney T. Hartnett 1963 THESIS This is to certify thatOthe thesis entitled An Analysis of'Factors Associated With Changes in Scholastic Performance Patterns presented by Rodney T. ertnett has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D Education ' degree in 6%??M Date Ju { 0.169 @v ABSTRACT AN ANALYSIS OF FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH CHANGES IN SCHOLASTIC PERFORMANCE PATTERNS by Rodney T. Hartnett The purpose of this investigation was to explore the relationship between selected variables and changes in the academic performance patterns of students during four years of college. The study was designed to differentiate between students whose performance pattern improved as opposed to those Whose pattern of achievement changed negatively. The basic purpose was to examine the factors thought to have association with the change in performance at the time the change occurred. The sample selected for the study was composed of 1,041 students who entered a large midwestern university as fresh— men fall, 1958, and were in attendance during each fall, winter, and spring term through spring of 1962, Subjects were classified into change groups on the basis of the difference between their performance predicted from the previous year's grade point average (determined by linear regression) and their actual performance. Any Rodney T. Hartnett discrepancy greater than a confidence band of .50 standard errors of estimate was used as the criterion for change classification. The subjects were also classified according to their achievement into high, middle, and low groups. The change groups were then compared on three types of experimental variables. These variables were: (1) ability and aptitude, (2) attitudes and values, and (3) behavioral characteristics. Used as measures of the aptitude variables were The Michigan State University Reading Test, The Collgge Qualification Test, and A Test of Critical Thinking, Form G. —fi—‘ For measures of attitudes and values The Inventory of Beliefs, The Differential Values Inventory, and Rokeach's Dggmatism Scale were employed. The behavioral variables consisted of degree of participation in extra-curricular activities, place of residence, the amount of time devoted to part-time employment during the school year, and a number of personal factors. Analysis of variance was used as the basis for determining the significance of the differences between the performance change groups on the first two types of variables, whereas chi—square tests were employed for analysis of the data per- taining to the behavioral variables. Throughout the study a (D rt '0 If?) (n f (‘7 PI‘ (I) m Rodney T. Hartnett .05 level of probability was used in determining statistical significance. The ability-aptitude and attitude-value variables failed to discriminate between the performance changers. An occasional F-value exceeded by .05 level of confidence, but no more than would be expected by chance. It was concluded that these variables were not associated.with changes in scholastic performance patterns. The behavioral variables, on the other hand, were more helpful in distinguishing between performance changers. Those making a positive change during the sophomore year, for example, were typically those who: (1) pledged and joined a fraternity or sorority during that year, (2) worked fewer hours at part-time jobs than negative changers, (3) lived in a residence hall (whereas negative sophomore changers tended to list a fraternity or sorority as their place of residence), and (4) participated less in extra-curricular activities. During the junior year, those making positive changes (as opposed to those making negative changes) were characterized by: (1) fewer changes of major field, (2) fewer students becoming "active" in fraternities and sororities, (3) less employment, and (4) less participation in extra-curricular activities. The behavioral variables associated with changes 12‘. Cd pronou still Rodney T. Hartnett in academic performance during the senior year were not as pronounced as during the sophomore and junior years, but still provided some clues regarding behavioral differences. Positive changers during the senior year more likely lived in residence halls, whereas negative changers more likely lived in fraternity or sorority houses. AN ANALYSIS OF FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH CHANGES IN SCHOLASTIC PERFORMANCE PATTERNS BY ,Cv‘, Rodney Tf Hartnett A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY College of Education 1963 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The writer wishes to express his sincere appreciation to Dr. James Costar, Chairman of the committee, for his encourage- ment and guidance. In addition, the author is grateful to Dr. Arvo Juola, Dr. Hans Toch, and Dr. Willa Norris, for valuable criticisms and suggestions. Especial appreciation is extended to Dr. Juola, who, in his role as Thesis Chairman, was of in- valuable assistance from the initial planning stages of the study through its completion. A portion of the data reported herein was gathered as part of a larger investigation of "Critical Thinking, Attitudes, and values in Higher Education," sponsored jointly by Michigan State University and the United States Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Office of Education. The writer is grateful to Dr. Paul Dressel for his permission to use some of this data, and to Dr. Irvin Lehmann for his many suggestions and thoughtful considerations. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENT . . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS . . . . . CHAPTER I. THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . II. III. Introduction . . . . . . Statement of the Problem Factors to be Examined . Definition of Performance Change Determining expected grade point Determining change classification The Hypotheses . . . . . Scope and Limitations of the Study Importance of the Study . REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE General Overview . . . . Attitudes, values, 0 O averages. The Behavioral Variables and Academic Changes in Scholastic Performance Summary . . . . . . . . . THE METHOD OF INVESTIGATION Definition of the P0pulation Classification of the Sample as to Change Instrumentation . . . . . The College Qualification Test . The Test of Critical Thinking, The Michigan State University Reading Test The Inventory of Beliefs . The Differential Values Inventory Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale The Experience Inventory . iii 0 Procedure for the Collection of the Data Procedure for the Analysis of the Data Form G and Scholastic Performance Tests of Aptitude and Academic Performance Page ii 23 23 26 34 Performance 41 52 55 57 57 58 58 58 6O 61 62 64 66 68 72 73 IV. E IV. ANALYSIS OF THE DATA. . V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION, AND BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . APPENDIX . . . . . . . A Description of the Sample . . . . Grade-point ranges . . . . . . . . . . Correlation between annual grade point averages . . . . . . . . . Standard errors of estimate . . Characteristics of the group . . . Hypothesis I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Male students . . . . . . . Female students . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions regarding Hypothesis I Hypothesis II . . . . . . Male students . . . . . . . . . . . . Female students . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions regarding Hypothesis II Hypothesis III . . . . . . . . . . . . . Sophomore change . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions regarding sophomore change Junior change . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions regarding junior change Senior change . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions regarding senior change . FOR FURTHER RESEARCH . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Purpose and procedure . . . . . . . Findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Implications for Further Research . . . . iv IMPLICATIONS Page 75 75 76 78 79 83 84 85 87 89 9O 91 95 98 99 100 108 110 118 118 124 127 127 127 130 132 135 139 142 151 QC LIST OF TABLES TABLE 10 Grade point average ranges for achievement groupings . . . . . . . . . . . . . Intercorrelations of annual grade point averages, male and female, for group maintaining continuous enrollment, fall, 1958, to Spring, 1962 . . . . . Grade point average discrepancy equivalents of the .50 standard error of estimate reported for sophomore, junior, and senior change points . . . Means and standard deviations on instruments used for ability-aptitude and attitudes-values variables, with comparative data for entire sample of students entering Michigan State University in fall, 1958 . . . . . . . . . . . . Descriptive break-down of students by behavioral variables during sophomore, junior, and senior year S O O O I O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Ability and aptitude mean scores of male students classified by freshman grade point average and direction of performance change during the sophomore year . . . . Ability and aptitude mean scores of female students classified by freshman grade point averages and direction of performance change during the sophomore year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Attitude and value mean scores for fall, 1958, testing: male students classified by freshman grade point average and direction of performance change during the sophomore year . . . Attitude and value mean scores for spring, 1959, testing: male students classified by freshman grade point average and direction of performance change during the sophomore year . . . . . . . . V Page 76 78 79 81 82 86 88 92 94 Ty“ 2’ T ”has“. 13 H (3" F4 (j) I LL J TABLE Page 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. Attitude and value mean scores for fall, 1958, testing: female students classified by freshman grade point average and direction of performance change during the sophomore year. . . . . . . . . 96 Attitude and value mean scores for spring, 1959, testing: female students classified by freshman grade point average and direction of performance change during the sophomore year. . . . . . . . . 97 Comparison of students classified by direction of sophomore performance change, level of freshman achievement, and applicability of personal factors during the sophomore year . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 Comparison of students classified by direction of sophomore change, freshman achievement level, and hours employed per week during the sophomore year 103 Comparison of students classified by direction of sophomore change, freshman achievement level, and place of residence during the sophomore year. . . 105 Comparison of male students classified by direction of sophomore change, level of freshman achievement, and activities immersion during the sophomore year 108 Comparison of female students classified by direction of sophomore change, level of freshman achievement, and activities immersion during the sophomore year. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 Comparison of students classified by direction of junior performance change, level of sophomore achievement, and applicability of personal factors during the junior year. . . . . . . . . . . . . lll Comparison of students classified by direction of junior change, sophomore achievement level, and hours employed per week during the junior year. . 113 Comparison of students classified by direction of junior performance change, sophomore achievement level, and place of residence during the junior year. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115 vi TABLE 61. 6,2. 20. 21. 22, 23. 24. 25. *3 OJ .‘b' ,‘d rhgltt a: TABLE 20. 21. 22. 23 243 255 26a Page Comparison of male students classified by direction of junior change, level of sophomore achievement, and activities immersion during the junior year . 116 Comparison of female students classified by direction of junior change, level of sophomore achievement, and activities immersion during the junior year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 117 Comparison of students classified by direction of senior performance change, level of junior achieve- ment, and applicability of personal factors during the senior year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 Comparison of students classified by direction of senior change, junior achievement level, and hours of weekly employment during the senior year . . . 122 Comparisons of students classified by direction of senior change, junior achievement level, and place of residence during the senior year . . . . . . . 123 Comparison of male students classified by level of junior achievement, direction of senior academic performance change, and activities immersion during the senior year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 Comparison of female students classified by level of junior achievement, direction of senior change, and acitivities immersion during the senior year. . . 125 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Test of Critical Thinking . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement of males on fall, 1958, College Quali- fication Tests . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Michigan State University Reading Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . 154 vii TABLE Page 9.2. 10.1. 10 2 10.3 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Test of Critical Thinking . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, College Quali— fication Tests . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 156 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Michigan State University Reading Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Inventory of Beliefs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 158 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Differential Values Inventory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159 . Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Dogmatism Scale. 160 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on spring, 1959, Inventory of Beliefs I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I o 161 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on spring, 1959, Differential Values Inventory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Inventory of Beliefs I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 163 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females of fall, 1958, Differential Values Inventory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Dogmatism Scale I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 165 viii 5;; m E; 11.1. Are ac": Be. 11.2, Aid; 'v'al TABLE 11.1. 11.2. Page Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on Spring, 1959, Inventory of Beliefs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 166 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on spring, 1959, Differential Values Inventory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167 _—s-. -w- r - Figure 3;, 1, Re 1 1‘ LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure No. Page 1. Regression of freshman grades on sophomore grades, illustrating achievement classifications and direction of performance change . . . . . . . . . l4 2. Grid used for weighting student activities and specific positions or functions in that activity. 70 3. Progressive mean grade point averages, male and female, for group maintaining continuous enrollment, fall, 1958, to spring, 1962 . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Per least i: has rec topic I ing out by soar C1112e: SC3-001: CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Introduction Perhaps no area of research in higher education -- at least in the fields of psychology, sociology, and education -- has received as much attention in the last decade as the broad topic referred to as "research on college students." Originat— ing out of pressures placed on the colleges and universities by soaring enrollments, tight budgets, and questioning citizens, inquiries into the four years of undergraduate schooling have increased rapidly. Joining with those already probing the area, psychologists and sociologists have recently recognized the vast potential for social psychological research studies of collegestudents. Consequently, we have seen a marked increase in the investi— gations of the environment of the college campus and its various subcultures. Along with this, there has been a natural growth of interest in research dealing with the personality of the individual college student, and especially the person— ality changes that take place during the undergraduate years, presumably as a result of some facet of the college experience. Prediction of college achievement, of course, has been studied with a number of approaches. Measures of scholastic aptitude, specific subject-matter ability, interests, socio- cultural backgrounds, high school performance -- all of these and others have been used as variables in attempting to pre- dict college performance. There have been a large number of studies dealing with over and under-achievers, as well as "drop-out" or retention and withdrawal of students investigations, characteristics of diverse curricular area majors, and the like. Limiting the discussion to research on college students‘ academic performance alone, it can be seen that roughly four categories of current "types" of research exist: (1) census— type studies, which generally concern themselves with per- centages, such as percentage of students withdrawing, per— centage of students graduating, etc., (2) autopsy-type studies, which usually are post facto studies in the sense that they study characteristics of students who have withdrawn or been dismissed, (3) case—study approaches, which concentrate exten— sively on small samples of selected students for various reasons, and (4) prediction studies, generally attempting to 3 relate selected admissions variables to success or failure criteria (51). Research findings in these areas will be further discussed in Chapter 11. They are listed here, however, to illustrate the particular dearth of information which typifies our knowledge of what factors are associated with patterns of academic performance during the student's years in college. The existing research on college students' academic perform- ance has concentrated on two points in the student's course -- early college achievement and the eventual record at graduation or withdrawal. This approach has overlooked the intervening performance pattern. This assumes that scholastic perform- ance patterns are stable, an assumption which is not necessar- ily true. Students are generally regarded as being high achievers, low achievers or "average" achievers. The possible deception in this standard classification is that the student may be all of these at different times in his undergraduate years. The "average" achiever, for example, may actually be one whose term-to-term pattern consists of peaks and valleys -- high performance one term, low the next term, and so on. Or he may actually be a high achiever, but one whose over—all grade point average is depressed by low performance during his initial terms, or by a short—term period during later terms. 4 The point being made here is essentially this: that while for some students the performance pattern in college is quite consistent from their freshman year to their time of graduation or withdrawal, this is often not the case, and that, in contrast, many students' patterns of attainment are characterized by gradual downward or upward trends or a "roller-coaster" up and down model. Consequently, to judge one's performance pattern by a cumulative grade average may be quite deceptive, for the process of averaging may actually conceal the most revealing index of the present level of achievement. Those familiar with and involved in higher education can readily recognize the importance of understanding more about the student‘s performance pattern as Opposed to a cumulative index of achievement. A student with an over-all grade point average of 2.5 might well be overlooked as one who possibly has academic difficulty, yet if it is understood that this 2.5 represents the average attainment, and his term-to-term pattern consists of a number of changes in his level of attainment, then further consideration should be given to this student's performance status. While this kind of performance analysis has been attempted in modified forms at several universities (56, 87) there has 5 been, up to this point, a failure to ferret out the factors which might have some bearing on inconsistent performance patterns. If it can be demonstrated that student X, who has a grade point average of 3.0, is nevertheless a student in need of some assistance because of his instability of perform- ance, the probem is only recognized. The question must still be answered: What factors seem to be associated with gross changes in college students' levels of scholastic attainment? Various studies have indicated a relationship between such variables as attitudes, values, and beliefs, to changes in one's curriculum (51, 54). These same variables have been demonstrated to bear some relationship to withdrawal and attrition rates (43). Certainly such variables as attitudes, values, and beliefs would possibly be associated.with sizable changes in academic performance. There has also been a considerable amount of research regarding the mutual association of various student activities (i.e., fraternities, student government, etc.) and various levels of attainment in college, eventual graduation, or subsequent withdrawal. Once again, such variables would con- ceivably be important to consider in any investigation of Chan es in one's academic erformance attern. P 6 The academic performance of a college student may simply be regarded as a specific sample of behavior. Like other bits of behavior, it is susceptible to change and is affected in various ways by different situations and previously held diSpositions to behave in certain ways. Unfortunately, however, too few recognize that performance patterns change, and even fewer have attempted to isolate any factors associated with such changes. Statement of the Problem The problem investigated in this research is the relation— ship between various selected factors and changes in students' academic performance patterns during four years of under- graduate study at Michigan State University. The factors to be studied include certain measured "personality" variables, measured indices of aptitude or ability, and various bio- graphical data pertaining to employment, the amount of participation in extra-curricular activities, place of resi- dence during school, and the like. It is hoped that the findings reported herein will be of value in better understanding some of the basic reasons re- lated to students' inconsistent grade achievement during their four years in college. While the investigator realizes that 7 mere association of various factors with irregular patterns of performance does not necessarily mean causality, it is at the same time recognized that exploratory studies must come first if we hOpe to isolate the conditions associated with academic inconsistency. Factors to be Examined The factors to be examined can be classified into three categories: (1) abilities and aptitudes, (2) attitudes and values, and (3) behavioral variables. The ability and aptitude factors may be further delin- incide with the chart on this page. It is important to keep in mind that the grade point average used as the indices of achievement during the sopho- mere. aVerage . junior and senior years was not a cumulative grade point It was the level of academic attainment achieved by ‘Ehe student during that particular academic year only. Utilization of a cumulative index would not have revealed 304 14 ZDHQMZ mem .mmoouw ucofim>mflnum mmmum>¢ ucflom momma mHOEonnom o.m ¢.m N.m o.m m.N 9N .v.N N.N o.N m.H via a...” NJ” o.H 8v m V 3 \ JJ m1 0! \ I h H V \ \ eSexeAv auroa apexs uemqsaxg 15 performance patterns, since realistic changes in the students‘ the inclusion of the past totals would either depress or raise the present level of performance, thereby disguising radical performance changes. The Hypotheses As the review of literature presented in the next chapter demonstrates, much of the existing research bearing relevance to this investigation is either vague, inconclusive, or even contradictory. Moreover, the concept of scholastic performance change used in this study has not been employed previously in Iresearch dealing with the three classifications of variables loeing analyzed. As a result, the hypotheses, although evolv- iJng out of the general review of literature, lack the tradi- ‘trional framework of a stable theory. Keeping in mind, however, ‘tliat this is essentially an exploratory investigation, it was <3<>nsidered necessary to define more clearly the direction of tirre investigation. Consequently, taking care to avoid the 1‘11111 form, the following hypotheses were used as guidelines 3501' the study: 1) It is possible to differentiate among groups of students classified by direction of academic performance change, on the basis of their scores on ability and aptitude measures. l6 2) It is possible to differentiate among groups of students classified by direction of academic performance change, on the basis of their scores on attitude and value measures. 3) It is possible to differentiate among groups of students classified by direction of academic performance change on the basis of the amount of their employment, their place of residence, degree of participation in extra-curricular activities, and various personal factors. Sc0pe_and Limitations of the Study Not unlike many research studies dealing with college satudents' academic performance, the present investigation was heunpered by several limitations. While recognized as factors tc>‘be considered when drawing inferences from the findings, ‘hcnwever, it is felt that the limitations listed are not Stufficient to offset the importance of the data presented. One of the first limitations concerns the sample. Only tkuzse students who remained in continuous attendance (except fOI"the summers) for four years following the term of their enr'Ollment were included. Obviously, students who withdrew frCHn the university were not among those for whom.it was POSEiible to compute the attainment patterns in the sophomore, junixor and senior years, depending on the time of their with- draWal, But neither were their levels of attainment used in 'U“3 regression equations used in arriving at predicted levels 17 of achievement. It was felt that inclusion of the grade point averages of those students who dropped out of the university would lend a spurious change pattern to those who remained in school for the entire four years. A second limitation is one encountered by numerous inves- tigators employing data pertaining to obtained measures of gpersonality. These measures, in this study, were used to (determine such personality traits as dogmatism, rigidity, txeliefs, and values. The typical approach to measuring these \narious characteristics is to elicit from the respondent some iruflication of his feeling or judgment on an empirically de- rtrved affective scale. There is no guarantee that this response is an honest one, nor, assuming that it is honest, is thueir any evidence that this felt attitude has any stability. Nevertheless, this is a limitation faced by nearly all inves- ti43ators engaged in research of this nature, and if it is to bereconsidered as a limitation, it certainly is not one which is peculiar to this particular study. There are other limitations concerned with the grades of the= students, the variable used to construct the entire study. PrCflDably most obvious is the problem of different grading Staruflards. The utilization of grade point averages for vanPious statistical manipulations sudh as part of a regression l8 equation, or simply a rough classification procedure as that employed here, makes the assumption of uniformity of grade assignment. However, what is work of "A" calibre for one ‘professor may only be deserving of a ”B" for another instructor in the same course. As a result, changes in the academic ;performance patterns of the students may possibly be the :result of different faculty standards, not any factor dealing vwith the student at all. Different curriculum standards (i.e., tuuequal standards in different academic departments) is a stnilar difficulty. The students who carry part-time loads introduce another type of limitation under the grading problem. It is possible tIHat changes in one's pattern of attainment may actually be tIue result of his credit-hour-load. Only grade point averages were considered here, and consequently those carrying one or tVKD courses were not separated from those carrying full academic schedules . Still another limitation having to do with the grade factuor is the problem of repeated courses. Students receiving unsEl‘tisfactory grades in courses have the opportunity of re- Peailing that course with the good probability that their grade Wil-l'be improved. No extra credit is given when the course is repea¢ed, but the honor points earned are substituted in place 19 of those deserved during the student's first time in the course. As a result, a student could conceivably go from a low level of achievement to a very high level, as long as his performance in the repeated courses is much better. Again, the change in performance pattern may actually be a function of :repetition of courses, rather than any of the variables being exendned in this investigation. These limitations regarding the grading factor, while lJending a proper note of caution to the interpretation of the rtesults, do not render meaningless the grade-based procedure ennployed. In spite of the recognized limitations, grade point axnerages still represent reliable evaluation and certainly I reunain our best estimate of student performance. Importance of the Study The importance of this study is best illustrated through coI'lSSiderations of the basic substantive and methodological in‘Plications. Analyses of patterns of academic performance in-lieu of a cumulative grade point average offers a different PrOCedure for evaluating student progress and the effects that different experiences have on this scholastic performance. SOWKB of the techniques employed for measurement of the be— lfifllioral variables (i.e., the Activities Index) also present 20 a relatively new way of viewing the student sub-culture. In these respects, the importance of the study basically lies in its method of investigation. It is important because it is suggesting a new approach to an old problem and denotes a further contribution of research to our body of knowledge. This study may not provide clear-cut information of practical mean— ing to anyone, but if it serves as a "springboard" for allied :research, then its importance is recognized. But its importance can also be illustrated through con- saideration of the implications this research holds for both tflne students and the university. While it is agreed that rwesearch need not be of a pragmatic nature to be meaningful, it: is also felt that an understanding of some of the appli- cartions of the findings may lend more significance to educa— t ional research . In terms of the student, analysis of factors associated ‘Wiifli academic performance patterns may yield discoveries having Valfiious implications. Students experiencing difficulty in mailitaining a stable pattern of achievement, but who are unalble to understand the reasons for their inconsistency. miglrt be better able to appreciate their personal situation. Exierting research, for example, indicates that participation in extra-curricular activities has little or no effect on the 21 scholastic achievement of college students (74). If, however, it is revealed that periods of peak activity in the various campus organizations are associated with a downward trend in the student's level of performance, then appraisal of one's outside-of-class activities may be pertinent. In terms of the university, the research related to students' performance patterns has multiple implications. Eketter counseling could be provided those students who are txeset with difficulties related to negative fluctuation of tflieir academic performance. If it is revealed, for example, tluat personality rigidity is a factor associated with students \vhuase pattern of attainment is somewhat of a "roller-coaster“ type, then it is conceivable that a more direct approach to resolving the situation could be available. Finally, those involved in student personnel administra— ‘ticnu might find the results of this study meaningful and fihelqpful as guidelines in the establishment of various policies regarding student housing, fraternities and/or sororities, Partlicipation in out-of-class activities, student employment and the like . A critical reivew of the literature related to this study Wil-lbe presented in the following chapter. The findings of .(y .1.“ 22 previous investigators will be reported and discussed. In Chapter III an account of the methodology of the study is presented, including a description of the sample, a discussion of the instruments used, procedure for gathering the data, and the processing and analysis of the data. In the fourth chapter the results of the analysis are presented, and the fifth and final chapter of the thesis contains the summary. CHAPTER II A REVIEW OF LITERATURE While it is true that research on college students has increased notably in the past decade, it is also true that there is a great need for more which is of high quality. This is especially true in the area of college students' academic performance patterns. As suggested earlier, the current literature is addressed primarily to the "pre—" and "post-" points, and tends to overlook completely the pattern during college. The present chapter is the result of an attempt to review that research which has some bearing on the thesis. It discusses both the findings regarding the "factors" under examination and also the various instruments used. General Overview Prediction studies and drop—out studies have proven to be the paths most often chosen by those interested in explor- ing the area of college students' academic performance. Although not directly related to an analysis of changes in 23 24 performance patterns, a number of these investigations shed considerable light on the over-all problem of student achieve- ment and many of the variables involved. Probably the most comprehensive report on student with— drawal at the time of this writing, is a census study by Robert Iffert (42). Sponsored by the United States Office of Health, Education and Welfare, Department of Education, Iffert surveyed nearly 150 colleges and universities in 1950. Foremost in his findings were the revelations that approximately 50% of a college's student body departs from school within four years after beginning, about 40% graduate "on schedule," and roughly 20%.go on to graduate from some college some day. In this regard, Iffert's study is supported by investigations of others (58,76). (College drop-outs apparently do not differ significantly in seu< (46,63,75), or age (18,77), but can be distinguished on tlue basis of certain socio—economic characteristics such as ferther's occupation (75), parents' education (64) and size 0f htnnetown (22). Almost all studies of drop-outs have re- Vealeua significant differences in high-school performance. earlfir college achievement and scores on various measures of aptiinide or ability, with the drop-outs being lower in all three instances . 25 Of the wealth of information garnered from drop-out studies, that which has the most relevance for the present investigation may well be found in the area of motivation. {That various motivational factors are extremely important in ‘the evaluation of patterns of scholastic performance is a fact lnighlighted by most of the studies which consider motivation. Iffert‘s study (42) revealed that of 1450 withdrawals, 48% of the men and 33% of the women report disinterest in their studies as a primary reason for dropping out. Freedman (24) and.Farnsworth (18) also report lack of motivation as being an important reason for withdrawal. Vocational motivation is even more obviously related to Ehzholastic performance. A number of investigators have shown, Iftor example, that students who have made definite vocational <2}ioices are more likely to be successful college students Eacademically (23,42,69,81). Generally, however, drop-out studies have been far from £3Eitisfactory in terms of answering questions regarding college 5students' ongoing performance. They have had some influence, ‘kKDwever, on our understanding of a terminal point in one's Collegiate career . 26 Attitudes, Values, and Scholastic Performance Not previously considered as factors having any bearing <3n academic performance, there has been a recent growth in ‘the amount of research attempting to relate attitudes and ‘values to academic performance in college. Heist and Williams (35), in a study of the freshman class at California Institute of Technology, attempted to determine some of the correlates of different levels of achievement within a relatively homogeneous group of high abi l ity students . Dividing the all-male group into three achievement gfiroups (high, medium, and low) on the basis of their freshman Year grade point average, they then obtained the students‘ Ssczores on various inventories, including the Omnibus Personal— -icty'Inventory, the Strong Vocational Interest Blank, The Ikillport-Vernon—Lindzey Study of Values, and two specific 13Giographical items relating to student interests and expec- t at ions . They found that the primary distinguishing feature among lihe groups was a tendency on the part of the high achievers ‘to value more highly a strong orientation toward inquiry, and 27 speculative and creative thought. Ehrlich (l6) explored the relationship between students‘ (degree of dogmatism and their academic achievement. Using IRokeach's instrument to measure dogmatism, the author hypoth- ‘esized that if dogmatism implied a "closed cognitive structure," 'then this would have some effect upon one's capacity to learn, independent of academic aptitude. The results confirmed Ehrlich ' s hypothesis . Lehmann (53) examined the relationships between various <:ognitive and affective variables to student performance in cuemmon courses. The cognitive and affective variables in tliis case were six in number: the Inventory of Beliefs, A Thest of Critical Thinking, Rokeach‘s Dogmatism Scale, Prince's JEMifferential Values Inventory (affective measures), and the C2<>llege Qualification Test and Michigan State University I{eeading Test (cognitive). The dependent variable of student IDeerformance was designated as achievement in the required jflreshman courses of Communication Skills and Natural Science, Elsa well as the first term grade point average. For the most part, Lehmann was unable to establish a 13'fluom ucmpsum unflufimflOB How pwlfi GHHO N 0H5 Hm 71 As an example, assume student X belonged to several activities during his junior year. According to his experience inventory, he was a member of the junior class council for the entire year, serving as a committee chairman for one term, and he was a reporter for the student newspaper for two terms. His activities index score would be calculated as follows: Member of Class Council = 2 points x 2 terms = 4 points Chmn. of Class Committee = 3 points x 1 term = 3 points Reporter for newspaper = 2 points x 2 terms é_4 points Total Annual Activities Index . . . . . . . =11 points It was found that the distribution of activities index scores obtained by this weighting procedure had a highly skewed distribution, with a large number of students receiving 0, l, or 2 points for an academic year, and gradually dropping off to a small number of students with high activities index scores. Consequently, no assumptions of normality or linear- ity were made with the scores, and they were analyzed with a non-parametric technique, as were the other non—cognitive-be- havioral variables. One of the obvious limitations of the Activities Index was its necessary reliance on memory. Students were asked to recall not only what activities they participated in and what functions they performed, but also what terms they did each. Nevertheless, the procedure was probably better than going to 72 the individual student records, since information of this kind is not complete and often inaccurate. Procedure for the Collection of the Data During freshman orientation week in the fall term, 1958, a battery of instruments was administered to 2,973 entering freshmen at Michigan State University. The test battery included The College Qualification Test, The Michigan State University_Reading_Test, A Test of Critical Thinking, the Inventory of Beliefs, The Differential Values Inventory, and Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale. Complete and usable test data were gathered for each of the 1,085 students who maintained continuous attendance for the next four years. In May, 1959, or one academic year later, a similar test- ing program was completed. At this time, The Inventory of Beliefs and The Differential Values Inventorijere administered, and usable data were obtained from 1,041 students who were to maintain continuous attendance. Finally, a third testing program was conducted at the end of the students' senior year in college in May, 1962. At this time, the students were administered The Differential Values Inventory, The Inventory of Beliefs, Rokeach's Dogmatism 73 Scale, and The Experience Inventory. Usable data were gathered from 674 students at this time. The annual grade point averages for the students were obtained from the cumulative summary records of the Record‘s Office of Michigan State University. These records of student attainment are recorded on a term—to-term basis with the cumulative information also indicated. Calculating the students' annual grade point average was simply a matter of adding the term totals for a given academic year, and dividing annual points earned by the annual credits carried. This calculation was done on the IBM 604, Operated by the Michigan State University Data Processing Center. Procedure for Analysis of the Data The students‘ scores on the various instruments of the test battery were coded and key-punched into IBM cards. The achievement information obtained from the Record‘s Office was already on IBM cards, and consequently all the data relevant to the study was processed by machine, with each card con- taining the student number, sex, and curriculum as the identification characteristics. 74 Because of the nature of the data gathered, it was impossible to use a single statistical technique for the analysis. Some of the data, for example, is discrete, and hence could not be analyzed by techniques which assume normality of the population distributions, while other vari- ables met the necessary assumptions for parametric analysis. It was therefore decided to use analysis of variance in examining the data relating to hypotheses one and two (i.e., the non-COgnitive-personality and the COgnitive variables), and chi-square for the data relating to hypothesis three (or the behavioral variables). The calculations necessary for the analysis of variance were performed on the IBM 604, while the facilities of the Michigan State University Computer Center were used for both determination of the scholastic change groups and the chi- square analysis for the behavioral variables (MISTIC). CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF THE DATA The data gathered by the procedure discussed in the preceding chapter was analyzed in various ways, depending on the nature of the data and variables in question. It will be the purpose of this chapter to present in detail the analysis of the data, as well as some of the inferences such analyses make possible. The analysis of the data will be presented in four parts: (1) a description of the sample of students involved in terms of the variables being studied, (2) Hypothesis I, (3) Hypoth- esis II,and (4) Hypothesis III. A Description of the Sample In Chapter I it was mentioned that students were classified into achievement groupings on the basis of their obtained grade point averages, by simply dividing them into upper, middle, and lower thirds of equal size. The resulting grade point average ranges are presented in Table l. 75 76 Thus, it can be seen that if a male student's first year grade point average was above 2.60, he would have been classi— fied as a high achiever, or if 2.16 or below, he would have been classified as a low achiever. TABLE 1: Grade point average ranges for achievement groupings. Classification Points Achievement Freshman Sophomore Junior Groups Male Female Male Female Male Female High 2.60- 2.70- 2.59- 2.71- 2.68- 2.82- above above above above above above Medium 2.17- 2.28- 2.19- 2.27- 2.26- 2.36- 2.59 2.69 2.58 2.70 2.67 2.81 Low 2.16- 2.27- 2.18- 2.26- 2.25- 2.35- below below below below below below The ranges differ for males and females (with the latter typi- fied by higher grade point averages) and from year-to—year. For example, a grade point average of 2.63 would result in a high achievement classification for the males during the fresh- man year, but only a medium achievement grouping for females. Moreover, the same grade point average deserved a medium achievement placement for both males and females during the junior year. 77 A better understanding of this situation might be pro- vided by Figure 3,which indicates the annual increase in grade point averages from year to year for both males and females, but with female averages always being somewhat higher. Figure 3 is located on page 78. A final description concerning the grade point averages is presented in Table 2, where it is shown that the relation- ship between annual grade point averages decreases as the time lapse increases. For example, for the females (below the diagonal) there is a .79 correlation between freshman g.p.a.'s and sophomore g.p.a.'s. However, the correlation between freshman averages and the senior averages drops to .56. It has been previously indicated that a standard error of estimate of .50 was used in selecting the performance changers. The rationale for the .50 standard error was presented on pagell of the first chapter. The actual grade point average discrepancies which resulted from this figure are presented in Table 3. Figure 3: 78 Progressive mean grade point averages, male and female, for group maintaining continuous enrollment, fall,1958, to spring, 1962. 2 2.70 ( '7E9’ 2.65 (2.63)”/” I” 2 .60 l/ I I” 2.55 I; l” 2 2.50 __________________ El '54) (2.49) (2.49) (2.50) .46) 2.45 2.40 Males .40) (2.39) Females—--- 2.35 2.30 (2.32)_ Fall '58 Fresh. Soph. Junior Senior TABLE 2: Intercorrelations of annual grade point averages, male and female, for group maintaining continuous enrollment, fall, 1958, to spring, 1962. Fresh. Soph. Junior Senior Freshman .73 .55 .54 Sophomore .79 .67 .64 Junior .64 .71 .70 Senior .56 .61 .68 (Male correlations above the diagonal, females below.) 79 TABLE 3: Grade point average discrepancy equivalents of the .50 standard error of estimate reported for sophomore, junior, and senior change points. Male Female Sophomore .1978 .1715 Junior .2064 .1840 Senior - .2001 .2010 Consequently, if a student's sophomore grade point aver— age was .1978 points above or below his expected sophomore level of achievement (or .1715 if the student were female) he or she would be classified as a performance changer. As the table reveals, a discrepancy of greater magnitude would be necessary for change classification during the junior and senior years. The size of the group being studied is varied. For analysis of the variables pertaining to the first two hypoth— eses, data were available for 655 males and 386 females. For analysis of the behavioral variables, data were available for 419 males and 255 females. These groups were further divided into achievement groups and change groups, so that in some cases, especially for the behavioral variables, the specific 80 number of students in a particular grouping became quite small. The specific group sizes are included in the tables presented in the analysis of the data pertaining to the hypotheses. Descriptive data pertaining to the variables used in the investigation are presented in Tables 4 and 5 which appear on pages 81 and 82 respectively. In later tables dealing with analysis of data pertaining to the hypotheses, mean scores revealing characteristics of the entire group are omitted. This information is presented here so that the reader has some awareness of the entire group's status on these variables before being divided by level of achievement and direction of performance change. Table 4 contains the means and standard deviations for the entire group‘s scores on the measures used for the cogni- tive and non-cognitive—personality variables, as well as the means and standard deviations found for the entire entering class in 1958 on these same measures. The sample being studied here is a part of the larger group, and therefore data for the larger group is presented for purposes of comparison. 81 TABLE 4: Means and standard deviations on instruments used for ability-aptitude and attitudes-values variables, with comparative data for entire sample of students entering Michigan State University in fall, 1958. MALES FEMALES 7 Present Entire Class Present Entire Class Sample Entering 1958 Sample Entering 1958 Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Ability and Aptitudes: C.Q.T. 132.35 24.33 126.46 26.28 122.63 23.49 116.47 24.65 Reading 28.35 6.02 27.04 6.40 29.39 5.36 28.14 6.37 Crit. Think. 32.84 6.98 31.51 7.18 32.54 6.51 31.29 7.18 Attitudes and values: I.B. 64.06 14.16 62.86 14.08 65.55 12.50 64.77 12.84 D.V.I. 35.01 6.84 34.67 6.93 33.92 6.99 33.59 6.99 Dogmatism 166.72 25.22 168.19 25.36 162.89 24.38 163.56 25.47 82 TABLE 5: Descriptive break-down of students by behavioral variables during sophomore, junior and senior years. (N=674) (N=664) (N—662) Sophomore Junior Senior ACTIVITIES : N % N % N % Much 210 31 244 37 181 27 Some 237 35 210 32 225 34 Little or None 225 34 210 32 256 39 100 101 100 Place of Residence Residence Halls 262 39 211 32 153 23 Fraternity or Soror. 98 15 111 17 109 16 Co-ops 87 13 94 14 86 13 Married Housing 83 13 83' 13 100 15 Off-Campus 134 20 165 25 214 33 100 101 100 Employment Over 20 hrs. per week 79 12 80 12 103 16 Between 10 and 20 148 22 144 22 120 18 Between 1 and 10 102 15 105 16 175 26 None 345 51 335 50 264 40 100 100 100 Personal Made 1st choice of major 89 13 34 5 7 1 Changed major field 161 24 84 13 29 4 Pledged fraternity or soror. 91 14 22 3 9 1 Became "active" in F. or S. 93 14 55 8 20 3 Became engaged 39 6 65 10 108 16 Got married 12 2 31 5 58 9 Became "pinned" 57 8 95 14 80 12 Began "going steady“ 85 13 68 10 39 6 83 Considering for the moment, only those students who are part of the present study, it can be seen from Table 5 that the males and females are quite similar with respect to the variables under consideration. Males appear to have higher College Qualification Test scores and higher Dogmatism scores, but other differences are minimal. When compared to the original entire entering class, it is evident that the present sample has higher mean scores on the measures used as indices of ability and/or aptitude. This is to be expected, since drop-outs and withdrawals are not included in the present sample of students. Table 5 presents the number and percentage of students for whom the various behavioral variables were applicable. Table 5 is interesting in several respects. The most notice- able characteristics of the entire group are: 1) Participation in extra-curricular activities drops off markedly during the senior year, 2) The number of students living in residence halls drops off sharply from the sophomore through the senior years, while the number of students living in off—campus housing increases, 3) The amount of weekly employment increases during the senior year, and 84 4) Engagements and marriages show steady increases from the sophomore through the senior year. It should be pointed out here that while the percentages reported under the activities, place of residence, and employ- ment sections always total 100%” such a total is not to be expected from the items under the personal category, since these items are independent of one another. Hypothesis I It is possible to differentiate among groups of students classified by direction of academic performance change, on the basis of their scores on ability and aptitude measures. After classifying the students according to freshman achievement level and sophomore direction of performance change (as outlined in Chapter I), the analysis of variance technique was employed to ascertain whether the differences between the change groups and their scores on the cognitive variables were large enough to be more than simply chance occurrences. The .05 level of confidence was chosen as the confidence level for the hypothesis, and was the critical level used throughout the entire dissertation. Data for the males and females were analyzed separately on the cognitive data because of the initial sex differences found to be associ— ated with their performance on these particular tasks. 85 Male Students Table 6 shows the results of the comparison of the change groups on the ability and aptitude variables for the males during the sophomore year. The cognitive variables were examined at the end of the students' second year (or their first change point), while the measures used to obtain the data were administered during the fall term of the students' first year in college. In this sense, then, the analysis is post facto. The low, middle, and high categories, indicates the student's standing relative to the rest of his group in terms of his academic achievement during his freshman year. This separation was particularly relevant when analyzing the ability and aptitude data, since academic performance and the instruments used as measures of a cognitive trait have been demonstrated to be related (47). Table 6 reveals the expected pattern of scores for the groups who are classified according to their first year scholastic performance. The highest scores on the ability and aptitude measures were obtained by those falling in the top third of the grade point distribution, followed in order by the middle third and the low third. This difference between groups was expected, and, in fact, was the major rationale for the division of the sample into this three- dimension type of arrangement. .0>onw @ manna CH pmumfia modam>lm mnu mo mdem> soaumufluo mgu paw .mmHMSUm same .Eoummum mo mmmummp .mmumsvw mo mEdm .COflDMHHm> mo mousom msfipummmu sump How Napswmmm on» Ca m.o paw .N.@ .H.@ moanma 0mm 11 1 1 .msoum mmcmfio ammo How A=Z=v wNHm mHmEMm mfiu op Homo“ msfidaoo mflu m>onw mommnusmumm msu CH nudges: pcm .mmcamdonm uswEm>mH£om any ou Momma .mmusoo mo :.£mH£= paw =.macpfiE: =.3OH= mpuo3 mgB .mummcmno m>fluamom u =+= swam mfiu paw .mmcmfiu on u :0: amen m£u .mummcmgo m>flummmn n :1: swam mflu .umma msflpmmm huflmum>flas opmum cmmflnoflzn.m.n.m.z .umma coaumofiwflam5o mmmaaoou.a.0.u .mcflxcflnB HMUfiUHuUn.B.U “poms on Hafl3 maonemm paw msoaumfl>munnm mafizoaaom mfiu .3OHHom swap moans“ mgu CH can .muom .mmanmu ucmsvmmnsm may mo mama How Hmpoe m mm m>umm HHHB manmu mwnB .mocwpflmsoo mo Hm>ma mo. on» pcommn unmoflmflsmflm mum mdem>1m w>onm mg» no 0:02 86 gv.a hm.mm OH.Nm vm.am mo.m om.mm m¢.hN mo.hN om.N mm.mm mm.m~ m®.¢m .m.D.m.2 mm.H mv.HmH 00.0va mm.hva 00.0 ma.mNH mo.mNH Hm.mNH fib.~ mo.HNH fim.¢HH mN.NNH .B.O.U mm.o m¢.6m mm.mm om.sm mm.o mm.~m sv.am mm.Hm m~.o ma.m~ m¢.m~ mm.mm .a.o m nose home Ammo m Ammo Aomv “may m ance lame name 2 + o I: + 0 II + o I. mem mannHz son .ummh muoeonm0m ozu mcflusp mmsmno madeHomumm mo soauowuflp paw mmmum>m assom mpmum cmEfimem >3 poemsmmmHo mucmpdum mama mo mmuoom same mpsuflumm paw muflaflnm no mamma 87 Within the groups, however, there are still no differences. That is, changes in performance pattern do n2£_seem to be related to any of the ability and aptitude measures employed in this study. Although there does seem to be a rather con- sistently higher mean score on all three variables for those whose performance has changed positively, this consistency is not borne out to be more than a chance occurrence at the .05 level of confidence. Female Students Table 7 shows the results of the comparison of the change groups on the ability and aptitude variables for the females during the sophomore year. As was the case with the male data, those in the top or high third of the grade point dis— tribution during the freshman year tend to obtain higher scores on the cognitive variables. In fact, in every case where a mean score is presented, the higher the achievement grouping, the higher the females' score on the cognitive variable in question. Once again, however, the differences occurring within the three achievement groupings are small. Though relative con- sistency seems to prevail, with the positive changers tending to score somewhat higher than the negative or downward changers, only one F-value exceeds the established critical I: .m>onm h mHQuB CH UmpmHH mmCHm>1m msu mo moCHC> CoHHwUHHo may pCm .mmuwsvm Came .Eopmmnm mo mmmummp .mmHMCUm mo mECm .CoHuMHHm> mo wUHCOm mCHUHmmmH sump How xHUCmmmm wan CH m.h pCm .N.h .H.h mmHQMB mom .moCmpHmCoo mo Hm>mH mo. wfiu pCome quUHwHCmHma 88 oo.N No.vm om.mm mv.mm w¢.N mm.mm mm.mm No.mm bo.H hm.mm mo.®N mo.mm .m.D.m.2 00.0 mm.mma oo.©mH Nm.H¢H No.0 hm.NNH mm.HNH mm.HNH mm.H m¢.HHH mm.moa Cm.COH .B.0.U so¢.¢ OH.mm mo.¢m gm.om hm.o HH.mm om.Hm on.mm No.0 mo.mm vm.m~ mm.hm .9.0 m Aasv Ammo AHmv m Ammo “was have m Ammo Ammo “sea 2 + o 1. + o .I + o I: mem WAQQHS 30A .HmmS mHoEOCQOm may mCHHCp mmCmno moCmEComnmm mo CoHuooqu pCm wmmnm>m uCHom mpmum Cmagmmum mg pmHmHmmmao mUCmpsum mHmem mo monoum Came mpsuHumm pCm mpHHHQC an mamas 89 level. In this particular case, the females in the high achievement group whose performance pattern for the sophomore year changed upward, have a significantly higher score on the test of critical thinking than those whose performance pattern changed negatively. Close examination of the difference in question, however, suggests that the significant F-value is actually a result of the variation between the students witnessing an upward change in grades versus those whose pattern "froze" or did not change during the sophomore year, and caution must be used in drawing conclusions from the significant difference. Conclusions Regarding Hypothesis I The measures used to gauge the students' ability to perform tasks of an academic aptitude or ability nature (i.e., a cognitive variable) were administered during the fall term of 1958 only. When analyzed regarding their relationship to changes in performance patterns occurring during the sophomore year therefore, the criterion is nearly a two-year—old index. Such characteristics have been demonstrated to possess a good degree of stability, however, and it seems safe to conclude, on the basis of the data examined, that the cognitive variables employed bear little association with changes in one's scholas— tic performance pattern. Nine male change groups and nine . ..~fl‘.|fi....l~r..Y-‘\i}n.c. ... Juli-41‘ M». II 90 female change groups were analyzed on three different measures, and although some consistency is apparent, statistical signifi- cance appears only once. Hypothesis II It is possible to differentiate among groups of students classified by direction of academic performance change, on the basis of their scores on attitude and value measures. The analysis of variance technique was again employed to analyze the data, and the procedure for grouping students the same as that discussed under Hypothesis I. Males and females 'were again considered separately, owing to previous research on college student populations using these same instruments (54). The instruments used to test Hypothesis II -— The Differential Values Inventory, The Inventory of Beliefs, and Rokeach's Dggmatism Scale -- were administered several times during the students' course of study. Consequently, analysis was possible more than one time. In the case of both the male and female groups, the performance changes taking place during the sophomore year were compared with non—cognitive- personality test scores recorded at two different points: (1) during fall term, 1958, or the beginning of the students' freshman year, and (2) during spring term, 1959, or at the end of the students freshman year at Michigan State University. 91 Male Students Table 8 shows how the sophomore year male change groups compare on the basis of their scores on the attitude and value measures taken during their first term in college. As was the case with the ability and aptitude data, differences between achievement groups are more apparent than differences occurring within achievement groups for the performance changers. For example, those in the high achievement group tend to be more flexible and/or adaptive than their counter- parts in the low achievement group (as reflected in higher mean scores on the Inventory of Beliefs), have a slightly higher traditional value orientation (suggested by their somewhat higher scores on the Differential Values Inventory), and be somewhat less dogmatic. Ferreting out differences for the performance change groups within the achievement groupings proves to be a more difficult task, however. None of the male F-values for the sophomore changers achieve a level of statistical significance. Not only is significant variation absent, but consistency is also lacking. Clearly, the measures of a non-cognitive-personality type are not related to direction of change in performance patterns for males, at least when comparing on the basis of freshman test scores, and sophomore performance change. 92 meHm>Im mfiu mo men-um .Co UMHMC> mo mmCHm> COHkuHHU mfiu pCm .mmumCUm CmmE .EOpmme mo momummp moudom mCHpummmH sump How prCmmm< may CH mwm pCm .N.m .H.m mmHQma 0mm .m>onm m magma 2H umumHH .mwumsqm mo .mHmum EmHumEmOQ w.£Ummxomu.m.Q.m UCm .huoqu>CH mmflHm> HCHHCOHMHMHQN.H.>.Q .mmeme mo hHOHCm>CHn.m.H .moCmpHMCoo mo Hm>0H "pom: ma HHH3 mCOHumH>mHQQm mums» .3oHHom pas» manna“ may 2H can .mumm mo. m3“ UCOMCQ “CMUHMHCmHm mum mmsHm>1m m>onm mnu mo 0Coz hm.H m¢.mmH Hm.moH mm.omH om.o mm.moH mo.mmH wo.va 00.0 mm.mmH mH.N>H mm.NhH .m.n.m mm.H m~.hm om.mm NH.mm mm.o mm.¢m mm.¢m NH.mm mN.H Co.vm MH.mm H¢.mm .H.>.Q mm.H mm.mo mm.oo vm.wm Hm.o v>.mm on.mo mm.¢o mm.o Co.mm mm.mm mm.mm .m.H m Amhv name “awe m Ammv homo Amen m “see news Avmv z + o I. + o I. + o 11 mem HAQQHZ 304 .mmm» mHoEosmom msu mCHHCp mmCmfio mUCmEHomumm mo CoHuumHHp pCm mmmum>m DCHom opmum Cmenmmnm ha pmHMHmmmHo mqupCum mHmE umCHumou .mmmH .HHmm How mmuoom CmmE mCHm> pCm mpCqupm .m mamCB . 93 Table 9 shows how the sophomore year male change groups compare on the basis of their scores on the attitude and value measures taken at the SEQ-0f their freshman year in college. Their scores in this instance, have been altered with exposure to the college environment for one year, where- as the scores used in Table 8 are indicative of the variable present when they came to the university. The make-up of the performance change groups is the same, of course. Here we find one difference large enough to be more than a chance difference. In the middle achievement group, the positive performance changers are significantly more flexible and/Or adaptive than the "freeze" group or negative changers. Although not verified by statistical significance, this variable also seems to favor the positive performance changers in the low and high achievement groups. That is, there seems to be evidence in support of the theory that those whose performance pattern takes a positive turn will tend to be less rigid or compulsive than those who exhibit a downward academic trend. ‘The Differential Values Inventory fails to differentiate between any of the groups significantly, and really portrays no hint of consistency or trend. 94 .m>onm m mHnma CH pmumHH mmCHm>lm gnu mo mmCHm> COHHmuHHU 03¢ pr .mMHMCgm mefi .Eopowuw mo mmwummp .mmumsvm no mean .COHDMHHM> mo mousom mCHpHmmmn dump How prCmmm< wnu CH N.m UCm H.m mmHQwB mom .moCmpHmCoo mo Hm>mH mo. mfiu pCOth quoHMHCmHma .Hmmm Cmfiflmmnm mo pr pm pwuwpmHCHEpm uoz .m.n.m mm.o s¢.am mm.m~ om.mm -.~ mm.om om.Hm mm.m~ Hm.o mm.m~ so.om ¢¢.mm .H.>.n sm.o ma.om Ha.mm m~.mo :Hm.m Ho.oo mo.mo no.8m om.~ oo.ao m6.mm mm.mm .m.H m Amss Ammv Ammo m Ammv Acme Amps m Chev Asmv “46v 2 + o .1 + o 11 + o I: mem HAQQHE BOA .umm> mHoEonmom 03» mCHHCp mmCm£U moCmEHomumm mo CoHuowqu pCm mmmum>m uCHom mpmum Cmfinmmum >9 UCHMHmmmHo mqupsum mHmE «mCHumou .mmmH .mCHumm How mmuoom Came mCHm> pCm opCuHuum um mqm COHCmuHHo on» pCm .COHUMHHC> mo wuusom mCHpummwu sump Com prCmmm< wfiu CH m.0H pCm .N.OH .H.OH mmHQma mmm .wUCmUHMCOU mo Hm>mH .m>onm 0H mHQma CH pmumHH mmCHm>1m .mmumsvm Came .Eooomum mo ammummp .mmuwsvm mo mesa mo. may UCOth quoHMHCmHm mum meHm>Im m>onm may mo mCoz 6 9 o~.o o~.ooa ov.HoH Hm.HmH mo.H em.oma «m.mmfl mm.omH om.o ov.~HH HH.H6H HH.moH .m.n.m mo.o Hm.mm H¢.vm mm.mm m~.o m~.¢m oo.¢m mm.¢m mm.a vm.mm ms.mm m¢.Hm .H.>.n 6H.o Hm.mo m~.ms m~.mm mm.o Hm.so os.ms H¢.s6 40.0 «m.om mH.Ho H¢.Ho .m.H m laws Ammo Ammv m Ammv Amvv Apes m Ammo name news 2 + o 1 + o 1. + o .l mem quQHz 30A .Hmwm mHoEonmom any mCHHCU mmCmflo mUCmEHOMme mo COHuomHHp pCm mmmum>w uCHom mpmum Cmfifimmum ma pmHMHmmmHo mqupCuw mHmEmm «mCHummu .mmmH .HHmm How monoum CmmE 05Hm> pCm mpCquufl ”0H mqméa 97 .m>onm HH wHQMB CH pmHmHH meHm>Im wsu mo waHm> CoHHmuHHU mflu pCm .mmumsvm meE .EOpmmHm mo mmmummp .mmumsvm mo mECm .CoHumHum> mo moHCom mCHUHmmmH mnmp How prCmQQC mfiu CH N.HH pCm H.HH mmHQmB mom .moCmprCoo mo Hm>oH mo. 0:» pCOhwfl HCMUHHHCmHm mum mmCHm>1m m>0Qm may mo oCoz .Hmmm CmECmmum mo pCm um pmumumHCHfipm H02 .m.n.m om.o mm.mm mN.om mo.om mm.N _oo.mN N®.mN mv.mm mn.o hm.om mm.mm 0H.hm .H.>.Q 00.0 00.0H 00.H0 00.HH 0H.H 00.HH H0.00 00.0s HH.H HH.H0 H0.0m 00.00 .m.H a idea 100V Ammv m Amme A000 Assn m Ammo Ammv 100V 2 + 0 1. + 0 I. + 0 I. mem MAQQHS 30A 1 1 111 11 1 1 .111 11 111 1 111 1 b P 111 111i .Hwoh mHOEOCCOm mgu mCHHCp mmCmfio CUCMEHomem mo CoHuUmHHU pCm mommm>m uCHom mpmnm Cmfinmwum an pmHmHmmeo mqupsym mHmEmm umCHummu .mmmH .mCHumm How mmuoom meE mCHm> UCC mpCuHupd .HH mamma 98 beginning of the freshman year. None of the differences are statistically significant, and consistency is totally absent. Conclusions Regarding Hypothesis II The attitude and value variables bear little relationship to changes in students' academic performance patterns in college. There are literally no associations between scores on such measures and change in performance for the females, and only slight association for the male students. Of the three instruments administered -- The Inventory of Beliefs, The Differential Values Inventory, and Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale -- it appears that The Inventory of Beliefs warrants some consideration as a possible index for this purpose. The obvious lack of relationship between the attitude and value variables and changes in performance patterns, prompted the investigator to omit further use of these variablesin analysis of performance changers. The analysis up to this point points out the random association between the students' scores on the instruments employed and their direction of performance change in college. 99 Hypothesis III It is possible to differentiate among groups of students classified by direction of academic performance change on the basis of the amount of their employment, their place of residence, degree of participation in extra—curricular activities, and various personal factors. Because the variables to be examined under Hypothesis III do not meet the assumptions necessary for use of a parametric statistic, chi-square was selected as the tool for analysis of the data in this section of the thesis. In only one case -- degree of participation in extra—curricular activities -- the male and female students were analyzed separately. Analysis of the data pertaining to the cognitive and nonécognitive- personality variables was made for only one change point. The present hypothesis was examined at fliree different change points —- the sophomore, junior, and senior years. It was the intention of the investigator to examine the association of behavioral variables with performance pattern change at a time of mutual occurrence. That is, behavioral variables were analyzed for relationship during the year the performance pattern change toOk place. For example, if student X changed negatively during his or her SOphomore year, then the behavioral variables relative to this student's sophomore year were examined. 100 Chapter III presents a more detailed discussion of the procedure followed in investigating Hypothesis III. It should be pointed out here that some of the items had to be omitted because of inadequate size of the sample to which the items applied. For example, students were asked to indicate the term of their getting married. During the early change points, this item did not receive enough checks to be worthy of analysis, whereas for the senior change point it became adequate. Some of the other items were not applicable for any of the three change periods in question. Sgphomore Change Table 12 shows how the sophomore year change groups, male and female, compare on the basis of their responses to a number of personal factors. The change groups have been divided into three achievement groups according to their cumulative freshman grade point average, and the table lists both the observed and expected frequencies, along with the chi-square value (9). Of the 24 chi-squares, four are of sufficient magnitude to be significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. Three of these, it is interesting to note, occur for one variable. All three change groups show a significant chi—square for the item asking whether or not they pledged a fraternity or A.wmmm£quumm mflu CH mm.H hm.o @h.H wm.m M . lmfi m amm.m ho.m w®.m AHV H Ame 0 Ave m “we 0 Ame 0H Ame 0H Aeavon Aosvms Amy 0 Amy 0H Ame m Ame 0 Amy 0 Amy m Amsvom AHHVHH Rev Amy Amy Amv on Amv Ame AHV .moCmUHHCoo mo Hw>mH mo. mnv pCOka quoHMHCmHm¥ mum mmHoCmCUmum pouommxm “pomOHoCm DOC was mmHoCvamuw pm>uomnov N CH NH.~_ mm.o @N.H mm.¢ Hm.o rho.h mm.m go.o nae 0 Ass 0 Amv m Ame 0 Ame NH Ame ma Aeneas Amy 0 10V m Aonvaa 10V CH va m “nevus AHHVmH A0HV0H Aosvofl Amy m Ame 0 Amy 0 Amy m ANHVHH Aoave Amavsm Amv OH mm.o mm.m mm.o mH.N hm.m smh.m swh.o wo.H va m Ame m Ame 0 “we 0 Ame ma Aoavma Amavvm A00 an 10H00H Amsvma Ame H x00 0. Assess “mavmfl Aomvmm inflame AHV H Hoaema Ame 0 “00 m Aoavo AHHVH AHNVHH Amy m zhpmmum mCHom: pwmmoum :mpmmum mCHom= Cmmmm guacaHm= 050 0 mm zpmmmmCm= mEsomm = ”NV.“ pom .- memowm .m Ho .m ummnmam HOnmE UmmCan Honda mmOCU pCm .Cmm> muoEOCQOm ms» mCHHCp mHOHUMM HCCOmHmm mo muHHHQMOHHmmm .UCmEm>mH£0m CMEfimmHm mo Hm>mH .mmCmnu mUCmEHomHmm OHOEOCQOm mo COHuomHHp ha pmHMHmmmHU mqupCum mo ComHHmmEou ”NH figmfifi 102 sorority during the sophomore year. The results, however, are inconsistent. For the low achievement group, those whose performance pattern went down during their sophomore year pledged a greek society less than expected. This same situation holds for the middle achievement group, but is altered somewhat by the high achievement group, where we see the downward changers also pledging more than we would expect. This occurrence becomes more understandable, however, if we are familiar with the achievement regulations of the greek society aspirants. Only those with grade point averages above 2.2 are allowed to pledge. Table l on page 76 reminds us that a 2.2 grade point average for the freshman males falls somewhere in the middle of the medium achievement group. The g.p.a. restriction would therefore result in fewer numbers from the medium achievement group and eliminate students entirely from the low achievement group. In the case of the females, a 2.20 restriction on pledging also eliminates a sizalfle portion of the low achievement group. The other personal factors which apply to the students during their sophomore year really give no evidence of the existence of a common thread or factor which might be con- sidered as being associated with a particular type of perform- ance change. 103 Table 13 presents the chi-square data, male and female, pertaining to the sophomore changers' amount of weekly employ- ment during the sophomore year. For the low freshman achieve— ment group, the amount of weekly employment seems to be a meaningful idex, although interpretation of the data is difficult. Of the positive changers, fewer than expected TABLE 13: Comparison of students classified by direction of sophomore change, freshman achievement level, and hours employed per week during the sOphomore year. Hours Employed Per Week Over 20 10-20 1-10 None X2 - 9 (8) 15 (12) 7 (7) 22 (25) H O 9 (13) 18 (20) 14 (12) 45 (40) 5.41 -+ 16 gle) 18 (l8)_ 9 (11)_ .35 (37) 34 51 30 102 - 4 (8) 21 (17) 10 (ll) 43 (40) M O 11 (9) 14 (19) 12 (12) 47 (43) 7.28 + 9 (6) 16 (14) 10 (8) 27 4(32) 24 51 32 117 - 6 (6) 15 (13) 19 (12) 30 (38) L O 10 (9) 22 (l9) l6 (17) 52 (54) 13.79* + 5 (5) 9 (12 ) 5 (l l) 44 (14) 21 46 40 126 (Observed frequencies are not enclosed; expected frequencies are in the parentheses.) *Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence 104 worked more than ten hours a week, while more than expected didn't work at all. The negative changers for that group, on the other hand, worked more than expected (had more students working over ten hours per week) and consisted of fewer students than expected who didn't work at all. Although extreme caution must be used here, this does suggest an association between amount of employment and direction of academic performance change in college. For the high and middle achievement groups, however, the trend is in the Opposite direction. That is, m2£e_than the expected number of positive changers work over ten hours per week, and fewer than expected did not work at all. Once again, as was the case with the fraternity and sorority pledging, the significant difference obtained with the low group may beaafunction of their poor freshman year achievement, rather than their Change classification at the end of the sophomore year. A comparison of the sophomore change groups on the basis of their sophomore place of residence is presented in Table 14. As indicated, the chi-square value for the high achievement group is high enough to be regarded as more than a chance occurrence. The largest amount of variation here seems to be contributed to those living in the residence halls and 105 TABLE 14: Comparison of students classified by direction of sophomore change, freehman achievement level, and place of residence during the sophomore year. Residence Sororities Married Other Halls or Co-ops Housing Off-Campus 2 Frats. X 11 (20) 16 (10) 10 (8) 8 (8) 8 (7) 33 (32) 18 (16) 10 (12) ll (13) 14 (12) 17.60* 37 (29) 7 (15) ll (11) 15 (12) 8 (ll) 81 41 31 34 30 28 (29) 18 (14) 10 (8) ' 8 (7) 14 (17) 30 (31) 13 (16) 9 (9) 9 (8) 23 (18) 4.43 27 (23) 12 (ll) 6 (6) 5 (6) 12 (13) 75 43 25 22 49 29 (31) 6 (4) ll (9) 7 (8) 17 (16) 47 (45) (6) 10 (13) 11 (11) 27 (23) 4.99 30 (28) 3 (3) 10 (8) 9 (7) 11 (14) 106 14 31 27 55 *Significant beyond .05 level of confidence. 106 fraternities (or sororities). It can be seen that a much smaller than expected number of negative changers lived in the residence halls during the sophomore year, whereas the residence halls housed a larger-than-expected number of students characterized by positive performance changes. Conversely, the fraternities and sororities housed a greater- than—expected number of negative or downward changers and a much smaller than expected number of positive performance changers. Differences for those living in the other types of housing appear to be negligible. The pattern witnessed in the high achievement group for the residence hall students and the students living in fraternities or sororities, can also be seen, though to a smaller extent, in the middle and low achievement groups. This suggests that rises in performance patterns seem to be associated with residence hall living, while decreases or negative changes are associated with fraternity or sorority living. These factors will be examined again at the junior and senior change points. The final variable examined for its association with sophomore change patterns was the degree of student partici- jpation in extra-curricular activities during the sophomore year. As explained earlier. the amount of time required for these activities has been used for the index of activities immersion. 107 Tables 15 and 16 present the activities data for the males and females respectively for the sophomore year. The classifications of "much," "some," and "very little or none" were derived by simply dividing the range of weighted immersion values into thirds which would most closely approximate an equal distribution of students. The weighting procedure for the activities index is described in Chapter III. For the males (Table 15), a significant chi-square value was obtained for the middle achievement group, Where we find the negative changers participating more than expected in extra-curricular activities (24 are in the "much" category, where only 14 were expected). The positive or upward changers, on the other hand, reflect an opposite pattern. Fewer than expected participated in the "much" end of the scale, whereas more than expected were found in the "none" category. The no-change group is characterized by a minimum of variation in the middle achievement group. For the females (Table 16), none of the chi-square values surpassed the chance level. It is interesting to note that both males and females participate in extra—curricular activities according to their achievement level, regardless of their present direction of performance change. Note, for example, that for both males 108 TABLE 15: Comparison of male students classified by direction of sophomore change, level of freshman achievement, and activities immersion during the sophomore year. Degree of Participation Very little Much Some or none X2 - 17 (15) 11 (ll) 10 (11) H 0 20 (19) 15 (l4) l4 (14) 0.54 + 19 (20) 16 (15) 16 g(14) 56 42 40 - 24 (17) 20 (20) 7 (14) M 0 15 (17) 20 (19) 15 (13) 11.10* + 9 (14) 15 (l6) 16 (ll) 48 55 38 — ll (9) l7 (14) 14 (17) L 0 ll (13) 19 (21) 30 (24) 3.62 + 10 .(8)_ 13 .112) 13 (14) 32 49 57 *Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence and females, the high achievement group is high on the “much" end of the participation scale, and low on the "very little or non" end of the scale, While the opposite is true for the low achievement group. Conclusions Regarding Sophomore Change The association of behavioral variables with performance change during the sophomore year was discovered to be slight but encouraging. Place of residence was the most promising variable, Where positive academic performance trends seemed 109 TABLE 16: Comparison of female students classified by direction of sophomore change, level of freshman achievement, and activities immersion during the sophomore year. Degree of Participation Very little Much Some or none X2 - 5 (5) 5 (6) 5 (3) H 0 16 (l4) l6 (l4) 5 (7) 3.09 + 9 ((19) ll (10) 7 (5) 3O 32 17 ‘_ - ll (8) 8 (9) 8 (9) M O 10 (10) 15 (ll) 8 (10) 6.05 + 5 ((7) 6 (7); ll (7) 26 29 27 «- 4 (5) 10 (8) 14 (13) L O 9 (7) .10 (12) 21 (19) 1.99 + 5 (4) 10 (8) ll (12) 18 30 46 None of the above X2 values are significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. to be associated with residence hall students, while those living in a fraternity or sorority were characterized by‘ downward patterns in achievement. Pledging a fraternity or sorority, amount of weekly employment, and degree of partici- pation in extra-curricular activities, all seemed to bear some relationship to the phenomenon of performance change, but the data regarding these variables was typified by inconsistency and great caution must be used in drawing conclusions regard- ing their value. 110 Junior Change The junior change groups were obtained by grouping students into high, middle, and low groups according to their sophomore grade point averages, predicting junior performance on the basis of the sophomore g.p.a., and placing them into change groups according to the discrepancy between their ob- tained junior grade point average and their predicted junior grade point average. A more detailed explanation of the grouping procedure and isolation of the performance change groups is presented in the first chapter. Table 17 presents the data relating to the association of various personal factors and students' academic perform- ance change for the junior year. When applied to the sopho- more change groups, pledging a fraternity and sorority seemed to be the key personal factor. For the junior changers, however, this variable does not apply. Four of the obtained chi-square values rise above the .05 level of probability and some of the tentative indications revealed by the data in Table 17 are: 1) Downward performance changers are more apt to change their major field. become "active" in a fraternity or sorority, terminate a stable heterosexual relation- ship, whereas, 1 .mOCOOHMCOO mo Hw>mH mo. 0:» pCOth uCMOHMHCmHma A.wmmm£qu£uumm mflu CH mum mmHOCmCUmHH pmuommxm upmmoHOCm HOC mum mmHOCOCUmHm po>nmmflov mm.mmim0 m 100 0 H40 0 .00.H WAHV m Ame 0H Has 0 0H.H 1H0 m 100 0H Hmv H .N0mmum mCHom=pmmmonm 0N.m_ E HH 2: m 7: m H00 :3 m 8va 23 NH 00.1. E 0 E 0 H3 0 $003....“ M mCHom: Cmmmm 00.0” HNV N Na N H: m 0H.0 H3 0 H8 0 H3 0 .30.: H3 0 E H E 0 =92:ng # mxonm mm.0 HHV 0 Hmv 0 Hoe 0 HN.N .HoHcH HNHch HNHV0H 0H.0 HHHVN HchNH Hoe mH =0mcch= wEmomm 00.0 Hoe m HHV H Ame s mm.0 HHV NH Hmv 0 Has 0 00.N Hoe 0 HHV H Hmv m 000m0am mEmOOm l s e u mN N Hwy 0 Ame 0 Hmv 0 :mo H Hwy N Ame 0 HHV NH No.0 Hoe 0 HHV m Ame m =m>Huom= mfimomm HH.0 HNV m Hmv N HNV m Hm.m HNV H HNV m HNV H 0N.H Hes H Amy 0 Amy N .m uo.m pmmcmHm :HN.H H05 0 H0H00H HHV NH 0H.0 HHV 0 Hmc 0 Hmc m N0.H Hova HNHvsH Hmc 0H uona meCmgu mm.0 HNV H HNV N HNV N 00.N Hmv 0 Ass N Hmc H 0H.0 Ame 0 H0c 0 Ave 0 Hone mmogu Nx + 0 Nx + 0 1 Nx + 0 .1 mem mannHz 30H 1 .HCO% HOHCCH any mCHHCO muouomm HMCOmHmm mo MHHHHQCOHHmmm OCm .uCoam>mHCOm OHOEOCaom mo Hm>mH uoHCdn mo CoHuomHHp >9 OOHMHmmmHO mqupCum mo COmHHsmEOU .omCmno OOCmEuomumm uhH mflmde 112 2) positive performance changers were less likely to change their major field, become "active" in a Greek social society, or break a "pinning'I or going “steady“ arrangement. Examination of the junior change personal factor table (Table 17) reveals, by way of supporting the points listed above, that negative performance changers changed their major during the junior year in greater than expected numbers and the positive changers changed choice of major field less than expected (this was significant for the high achievement group); that they became “active" in fraternities or sororities more than expected while the positive changers did so less than expected (significant for the middle achievement group): that they brdke “pinnings” and "steady" relationships more often than expected, while the positive changers did not (signifi— cant for low and middle achievement groups). Consistency seems to be present for the significant vari- ables discussed. Although in each instance a significant chi-square value was achieved in only one of the three achieve- ment groupings, similar results, though not as large, were usually the case in the other achievement levels. 113 Data pertaining to amount of weekly employment as a variable associated with junior year academic performance change is presented in Table 18. As was the case with the sophomore changers, they are classified according to achieve- ment grouping (based on sophomore grade point average), direction of performance change, and amount of hours employed per week during their junior year. TABLE 18: Comparison of students classified by direction of junior change, sophomore achievement level, and hours employed per week during junior year. Hours Employed Per Week Over 20 10 - 20 1 - 10 None X2 - 12 (9) 12 (13) 10 (9) 27 (28) H O 12 (12) 16 (18) 14 (13) 42 (39) 3.26 + 9 (10) 20 (15) 10 (ll) 33 £33) 33 —48 34 102 - 11 (9) 15 (18) 10 (10) 42 (39) M O 12 (10) 17 (18) 13 (10) 37 (40) 5.28 + 5 (8) 19 (14) 6 (8) 33 (32) 28 51 29 112- — lb (5) 9 (12) 10 (11) 32 (32) L 0 5 (7) 14 (18) 21 (17) 52 (49) 13.80* + 4 (6) 22 (14) ll (13) 37 (39) 19 45 42 121 *Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. For the low adhievement group, a significant chi-square 'value was obtained. Most of the O—E differences contributing 114 to this large X2 value seem to occur in the negative and positive change groups. Here we see the downward changers having a greater-than-expected number of students working over 20 hours per week and the upward changers having fewer than expected working over 20 hours per week. It can also be shown that this pattern is the same for all achievement groupings, although the deviation from the expected frequencies is not as great in those cases. Once again this suggests that the more hours a student is employed during the academic period, the greater is the likelihood that this student will witness a downward change in his or her academic performance pattern. At this point, however, such a conclusion cannot be warranted, and further analysis seems to be called for. A comparison of the junior changers on the basis of their place of residence during their junior year is presented in Table 19. None of the obtained differences were large enough to be attributable to more than chance occurrences. In each of the achievement groups, those who lived in the residence halls seemed to experience positive performance changes more often than expected, while those living in other off-campus living units placed a fewer than expected number in this category. In any case, the over—all variation was not TABLE 19: 115 Comparison of students classified by direction of junior change, sophomore achievement level, and place of residence during the junior year. Residence Frat. or Married Other Halls Sorority Co-Op Housing Off—Campus X 17 (19) 16 (12) 9 (8) 7 (7) 12 (14) 25 (26) 16 (16) 11 (ll) 8 (10) 24 (19) 6.47 26 (22) ll (14) 9 (9) 12 (8) l4 (16) 68 43 29 27 50 21 (21) 15 (15) ll (10) 9 (10) 22 (18) 20 (22) 16 (15) 10 (10) 11 (11) 22 (19) 4.68 21 (17) 13 (12) 8 (8) 11 (8) 10 (15) 62 44 29 31 54 20 (21) 7 (6) 12 (9) 8 (6) 14 (16) 34 (32) (9) 11 (14) 10 (10) 28 (24) 3.11 27 (26) 8 (7) 13 (11) 7 (8) l9 (19) 81 24 36 25 61 None of the above X2 values are the .05 level of confidence. significant beyond 116 sufficient to warrant any conclusions regarding the positive or negative effects of any of the living units being con- sidered. Tables 20 and 21 contain the data regarding the amount of participation in extra-curricular activities during the junior year. Table 20 presents this information for the male students, and Table 21 for the females. For the male high achievement group a significant difference was found, a difference which seems to be due, once again, to the variation occurring in the positive and negative change groups. TABLE 20: Comparison of male students classified by direction of junior change, level of sophomore achievement and activities immersion during the junior year. Degree of Participation very little Much Some or none x2 — 20 (13) 6 (12) 9 (9) H O 21 (23) 22 (20) 16 (15) 10.63* 4— 10 4(14) l7g_(lg) 9 .(9) 5T 45 34 - 18 (l7) l4 (l4) l7 (16) M 0 l6 (14) 12 (12) 12 (13) 1.03 4- 13 (15)_ 14 (13) 16 (14) 47 40 45 - 15 (14) 9 (10) 14 (12) L 0 20 (21) 17 (15) 18 (17) 1.08 + 20 (18L 15 L14) 14 (15) 55 41 46 H *Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. 117 TABLE 21: Comparison of female students classified by direction of junior change, level of sophomore achievement, and activities immersion during the junior year. Degree of Participation very little Much .Some or none X2 -- 15 (10) 5 (7) 6 (8) H O 12 (10) 5 (6) 8 (7) 8.09 + 10 _(l5)_ l3 (9) 13 (ll) 37 23 27 - 12 (11) 9 (9) 8 (7) M - 0 16 (15) 12 (12) ll (10) 0.59 + 7 (7) 8 (6). 5 ¥(5) 35 29 24 - — 7 (5) 8 (8) 8 (9) L 0 8 (8) 14 (13) 15 (14) 1.48 + 4 (5) 10 (9) 11 (10) 19 32 34 2 *None of the above X values are significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. For every level of achievement in both the male and female tables (Tables 20 and 21), it can be seen that the negative performance changers participated in greater—than- expected numbers in activities at the "much” end of the continuum, while in every case but one, the positive perform- ance changers were represented in this category less often than expected. This observed minus expected discrepancy is great enough to result in a significant X2 value for only the male high achievement group, but it is nevertheless present in every case._ 118 The findings regarding degree of participation in extra- curricular activities are in keeping with the findings at the end of the sophomore year. In both of these cases, there seems to be a tendency for the negative performance change to be associated with a greater—than-expected frequency in the “much” participation category. The opposite pattern is con- sistently found for the positive performance changers. Conclusions Regarding the Junior Change On the basis of the data presented regarding the associ- ation of various behavioral variables with academic perform- ance change during the junior year, these trends seem to be present: in comparison with the positive changer, the negative performance changers are more apt to change their major during the year, more likely to become active in a fraternity or sorority, terminate a stable, heterosexual relationship, participate heavily in extra-curricular activities, and hold a job requiring over 20 hours of weekly employment. Senior Chang§_ The senior change groups were obtained by grouping students into high, middle, and low groups according to their junior grade point averages, predicting senior performance on the basis of this level of attainment, and placing them into 119 change groups according to the discrepancy between their obtained senior grade point average and their predicted senior average. A more detailed explanation of the grouping procedure and isolation of the performance change groups is presented in the first chapter. Table 22 contains the data regarding the association of various personal factors with students' academic perfonnance change during the senior year. The first obvious difference between this type of analysis for the senior change group as opposed to the change groups for both of the first two years, is the reduction in the number of personal variables which apply. Sudh factors as "changed major field," "made first choice of major field," "pledged fraternity or sorority," and "became active in a fraternity or sorority," have all lost application to the students during their senior year in college. On the other hand, such items as "became ‘pinned,'" and "became engaged" are characterized by a marked increase in applicability (as evidenced by larger number of students checking them) and the item “got married" now enters the analysis for the first time. For the middle achievement group there is a significant difference on the becoming engaged factor, where fewer than expected became engaged for the negative change group .mocwowmcoo mo Hm>ma m0. 039 020%09 HGMUHMHGmHms RN 2: a E N E m mH.H x3 4 E m as o 34 E m H3 H E m 163$ maHow= Gmmmm 4N.¢ Ame m Amy 4H Hos m H¢.o HHV 6 Amy HH Hos m HH.¢ HoHVOH HOHVmH HHV 4 .6mcaHm. oamomm m Ho.m Hmv m HoHVHH Hoe oH HH.o Has 4 Hmv m Hmv m mm.H Ame m Hoe 6 Ave H anuumz How mo.H HNHVNH HoHVmH HoHVOH me.H Hmv m AHHVNH HHHVo No.4 ANHVHH HNHVmH Has 6 nmmmmcm mfimomm mx + o .l mx + 0 II «x + 0 II mon mAQQHS 304 .Hmw> Hoacmm mgu msHHSG muouomm anaemumm mo hnflaanmofiammm 0cm .ucmEm>mN£Um Hoacsm mo Hwbwa Reason «0 sofluomnao ma omHMHmmmHo mucmosum mo confiummfiou .mmsmnu mUGMEHowumm “NN mflmdfi 121 and more than expected did so for the "freeze" or no change group. This can hardly be interpreted as evidence that failure to establish stable heterosexual relationships leads to negative performance changes, however. For both the high and low achievement groups, the negative changers are typified by greater-than-expected numbers getting married and beginning "going steady" relationShips. The association of these factors, then, seems to be random and sheds no light on the identifi- cation of the various types of academic performance changers, at least for those changes occurring during the senior year. Comparison of the senior change groups on the basis of amount of weekly employment during the school year is presented in Table 23. This variable proved to be a promising one When applied to the sophomore and junior changers, but it yields no meaningful index of association when analyzed for the senior changers. As noticed before, the negative changers, regardless of achievement level, seem to work over 20 hours per week more often than expected, while the opposite is true of the positive performance changers. This consistency is encouraging, but, nevertheless, the differences obtained cannot be claimed to be more than chance occurrences at the .05 level of probability. It can also be pointed out that the relationship between level of achievement and employment still 122 TABLE 23: Comparison of students classified by direction of senior change, junior achievement level, and.hours of weekly employment during the senior year. Hours Employed Per Week Over 20 10 - 20 l - 10 None X2 - 11 (9) 10 (10) 15 (14) 23 (24) H o 14 (14) 15 (16) 20 (21) 39 (36) 1.11 + 12 (12)_ 16 (14) 19 (18L 3o (31) 37 41 54 92 — 15 (12) 16 (14) 17 (20) 33 (33) M o 13 (12) 14 (14) 21 (20) 34 (34) 2.76 + 7 (9) 9 (10) 18 (15) 27 (25) 35 39 56 94 - 10 (8) 13 (10) 18 (17) 16 (20) L o 12 (11) 14 (14) 22 (23) 31 (28) 3.35 + _9 (11) 13 (14) 25 (2;) 31 (2g 31 4o 65 78 None of the above X2 values are significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. holds, with the higher achievement group working more than the middle and low achievement groups Who follow in that order. How place of residence is associated with one's senior year performance change is presented in Table 24. The 21.07 chi-square value obtained for the high achievement group is significant beyond the .05 level of confidence, and is in agreement with the nature of the differences reported for both the sophomore and junior change groups. For every negative change group, regardless of achievement level, there is a 123 TABLE 24: Comparison of students classified by direction of senior change, junior achievement level, and place of residence during the senior year. Residence Frat. Married Other Off- Halls Soror. Co-ops Housing Campus X - 12 (14) 17 (9) 9 (7) ll (9) 10 (18) H 0 18 (21) 12 (14) 10 (11) 12 (14) 36 (27) 21.07* + 24 (18) 7 (12) 9 (9) 1,3 (12) 24 (24) 54 36 28 36 7O — 17 (18) 16 (14) 9 (9) 8 (10) 31 (27) M 0 19 (18) 15 (15) 10 (19) 10 (10) 28 (28) 3.06 + 14 (13) 10 (ll) 8 (j) ll (7)_» 18 (20) 50 41 27 29 77 - 12 (13) 13 (8) 10 (8) 7 (9) 15 (17) L 0 17 (18) ll (11) 10 (11) 12 (12) 29 (24) 7.26 + 20 (17) 8 (ll)_ 11 (ll) 16 (12) 23 (24) 49 32 31 35 67 *Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. smaller than expected number of students who lived in the residence halls, and for every positive change group, regard— less of the level of achievement, there is a larger than expected number of students who lived in the residence halls. This pattern is reversed for those who spent their senior years residing in a fraternity or sorority, and, once again, this finding is in keeping with.what was reported for the sophomore and junior changes. Those living in married housing are also found in the positive change groups more often than expected. 124 For the high achievement group, the largest O-E discrep- ancy is found for those living in “other off-campus” housing and occupying the "freeze" or no-change group. Review of the tables for the sophomore and junior change groups will show that those living in other off-campus housing consistently placed more than the expected number of students in the "freeze“ or no—change category. Stability of academic perform- ance seems to be their characteristic in this regard. Tables 25 and 26 present the data regarding the associ— ation of participation in extra-curricular activities and direction of academic performance change, for males and females respectively. None of the obtained chi—square values surpass the .05 level of probability. Conclusions Regarding Senior Changg_ The factors found to be associated with changes in scholastic performance patterns during the senior year were fewer in number than those associated with sophomore and junior year performance changes. One‘s place of residence seemed to be the best index, Where it was noticed that resi— dence hall living was associated with more than the expected number of positive performance changers, fraternity and/or sorority living during the senior year was associated with more than the expected number of downward performance 125 TABLE 25: Comparison of male students classified by level of junior achievement,direction of senior academic performance change, and activities immersion during senior year. . 2 Much Some Very Little X - 14 (12) 13 (13) 11 (11) H 0 14 (16) l7 (17) 18 (15) 1.24 + 20 (18) 20 (191. 16 g(11) 48 50 45 — T7 (12) 12 (14) 17 (18) M 0 13 (13) 14 (15) 22 (19) 6.97 + 6 (}0)_fi_ 18 (13);f l6 (16) 36 44 55 — “7 (7) 14 (12) 16 (16) L 0 1J. (11) l6 (16) 25 (23) 0.78 + 11 4(9) l4 (14), 21 (21)_ 29 44 62 None of the above X2 values are significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. TABLE 26: Comparison of female students classified by level of junior achievement, direction of senior change, and activities immersion during the senior year. Very little 2 Much Some or none X - 9 (6) 6 (7) 6 (7) .H 0 11 (11) ll (l3) l7 (14) 4.73 +- 4 (6) 10 (7); 7 (7) 24 27 30 — 9 79) 13 (14) 13 (11) M 0 8 (8) 14 (13) ll (11) 0.54 + 6 451 9 18) 6 (D 23 36 '30 1. - 7 (5) 6 (6) 7 (8) I. 0 7 (7) 9 (8) ll (11) 1.63 + '7 (8) 9 (9) l6 (13) 21 24 34 None of the above X2 values are significant beyond .05 level of confidence. 126 changers, and those living in other off-campus units were characterized by achievement stability. One's involvement in extra-curricular activities, amount of employment during the school year and other personal variables seemed to be of little utility in ferreting out those factors which might have some relationship to dips and rises in the scholastic performance patterns of college students. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION, AND IMPLICATIONS FOR FURTHER RESEARCH Summary Purpose and Procedure The purpose of this investigation was to explore the relationship between selected variables and changes in the academic performance patterns of students during four years of college. The study was designed to differentiate between students whose performance pattern improved as opposed to those whose pattern of achievement changed negatively. Students whose patterns of performance did not change at all were also compared. The basic purpose was to examine the factors thought to have association with the change in performance at the time the change occurred. If, for example, a student's performance changed negatively during the junior year, then "factors" having relevance for that student during the junior year were examined. The sample selected for the study was composed of 1,041 students who entered Michigan State University as freshmen fall term, 1958, and were in attendance during each fall, 127 128 winter, and spring term through spring term of 1962. Subjects were classified into change groups on the basis of the difference between their performance predicted from previous year‘s grade point average (determined by linear regression) and their actual performance. Any discrepancy greater than a confidence band of .50 standard errors of estimate was used as the criterion for change classification. Since the instruments used for measures of the cognitive variables have been demonstrated to correlate highly with academic achievement, the groups were further divided into high, middle, and low achievement groups. This procedure for determination of the performance change groups was con- ducted separately at the beginning of the sophomore, junior, and senior years. As a result, there were nine groups -— three change groups at each of the three levels of achieve- ment, analyzed at the end of each academic year. The change groups were then compared on three types of experimental variables. The variables were: (1) ability and aptitude, (2) attitudes and values, and (3) behavior. Used as measures of the ability and aptitude variables were The Michigan State University Reading Test, The College Qualification Test, and A Test of Critical Thinking, Form G. For measures of attitudes and values The Inventory_9f Beliefs, 129 The Differential values Inventory and Rokeach's Dogmatism Scale were employed. The behavioral variables were measured by an experience inventory developed by the investigator. The instruments used as measures of ability and aptitude were administered only at the beginning of the freshman year. The instruments used as measures of attitudes and values were administered at the beginning of the freshman year (fall, 1958) and at the end of the freshman year (spring, 1959), while the instrument used to gather information regarding the behavioral variables was administered only during the spring term of 1962, Which was the last term in school for students who had progressed at a normal rate. Analysis of variance was used as the basis for determining the significance of the differences between the performance change groups on the first two types of variables, whereas chi-square tests were employed for analysis of the data per- taining to the behavioral variables. Data regarding the ability-aptitude and attitude-value variables were analyzed separately for males and females, while the data pertaining to the behavioral variables were usually not divided accord- ing to sex. Throughout the study a .05 level of probability was used in determining statistical significance. 130 Findings The instrumentsemployed as measures of ability and aptitude failed to discriminate between male students classi- fied according to level of achievement and direction of performance change, and in only one instance differentiated between these groups for the females. Marked differences were revealed between achievement groupings —— the high achievement group recorded consistently higher cognitive scores than the middle or low achievement groups -— but not between performance changes within a given achievement group. This lack of association is true also for the attitude and value variables. There were no significant F-values forthcoming from the analysis for the females. and the association between such factors and change in academic performance was suggested on only one occasion for males. In addition to no differences between change groups, differ— ences between achievement levels were also lacking for the personality measures. The behavioral variables were more helpful in distinguish— ing between performance changers. Those making a positive change during the sophomore year, for example, were typically those who: (1) pledged and joined a fraternity or sorority during that year, (2) worked fewer hours at part—time jobs 131 than negative changers, (3) lived in a residence hall (where- as negative sophomore changers tended to list a fraternity or sorority as their place of residence), and (4) participated less in extra-curricular activities. During the junior year, those making positive changes (as opposed to those making negative changes) were characterized by: (l) fewer Changes of major field, (2) fewer students becoming "active“ in fraternities and sororities, (3) less employment, and (4) less participation in extra—curricular activities. The behavioral variables associated with changes in academic performance during the senior year were not as pronounced as during the sophomore and junior years, but still provided some clues regarding behavioral differences. Positive changers during the senior year more likely lived in residence halls, whereas negative changers more likely lived in fraternity or sorority houses. 0f the three “types“ of variables then, it was found that the behavioral variables were most frequently associated with academic performance changes of college students. 132 Conclusions It seems evident that, of the variables analyzed in this investigation, some show consistent trends of association with changes in scholastic performance patterns, While others are not associated in any discernible way. Measures of ability and/or aptitude and measures of dogmatism and values all seem to be typified by this lack of association. Though in some cases related to academic achieve- ‘mgntJ they seem to be completely unrelated to changes in scholastic performance. Although several F—values obtained from the analyses of variance did reach a level exceeding significance at the .05 level of confidence, the infrequency and inconcistency indicate that these associations are probably random and merely chance occurrences. The only variable which might be an exception to this is the variable of beliefs. Male students in the positive performance change groups con- sistently recorded higher mean scores on the Inventory of Beliefs. This suggests that the positive changers are those 'with less rigid personality structures and that this flexi— bility is somehow related to positive changes for males. The differences reported, however, are not of such a magnitude to be regarded as statistically significant differences, and 133 it must be concluded that, on the basis of this study, vari- ables of attitude and value or ability and aptitude nature (as described in this investigation) are 22£_associated with changes in scholastic performance patterns of college students. Behavioral variables, however, show considerable associ- ation with scholastic performance change. The type of behavioral variables associated with changes varies from year to year and, in some cases, consistency is lacking. Neverthe- less, the behavioral variables certainly warrant consideration as factors being associated with college scholastic perform- ance changes. Some of the conclusions regarding the behavioral variables which seem to be suggested on the basis of the data are: 1) Emplgyment: The amount of weekly employment during school is consistently greater for the high achievement group and, except for the sophomore changers, seems to be associated with negative changes in scholastic performance. That is, the more time one devotes to a part-time job While in school, the greater the likelihood that that student's grade point average will drop. 2) 3) 134 Place of Residence: Dormitory or residence hall living appears to be associated with positive changes in achievement, while fraternity or sorority living seems to be associated with negative changes in academic performance. For each of the three years and for each of the three achievement levels, the negative changers live in fraternity or sorority housing in greater-than-expected numbers, while the positive “" {nu-43' ”I." Q ' changers live in residence halls more often than expected. Associations are not as obvious or con- sistent for the other types of housing. but it does seem that married housing dwellers are typified by positive changes in academic performance, while off— campus students are found in the negative change category more frequently than expected. Activitievammersion: The degree of involvement in extra-curricular activities, while not as consistent as the place of residence variable, apparently does have some associ— ation with change in scholastic performance. Those whose grade point average changed negatively were consistently involved in extra-curricular activities 135 more often than expected, while positive changers were characterized by a lesser degree of activities immersion. 4) Personal Factors: The association between scholastic performance change and the personal factors examined in this investigation vary for each of the sophomore, junior, and senior years. During the sophomore year, there is a significantly greater likelihood that those who pledged a fraternity or sorority will be positive changers. During the junior year, this factor loses its applicability, but becoming ”active" in a fraternity or sorority takes its place and seems to be associated with negative changes. Junior year negative changers are also more likely to change their major field. These personal factors all lose their applicability during the senior year, where one finds little or no association between scholastic performance change and the personal factors considered. Discussion Out-of-class activities have often been overlooked as factors possibly influencing the scholastic attainment 136 patterns of college students. Organized student activities have generally been encouraged on the basis of the socialization value they hold for the student; part-time employment has been generally accepted as vitally necessary for many, with the apparent assumption made that it seldom affects academic achievement; and the many personal factors affecting each student have usually been regarded as research possibilities only for those interested in group dynamics or other forms of social psychology. The few attempts that have been made to explore any relationships that might exist between scholastic performance and the above variables have generally reported no negative effects of any of these factors. The findings of this investigation suggest a some- What different conclusion. Exploring this from the standpoint of changes in patterns of scholastic performance (rather than a cumulative grade point index), the data leads one to the hypothesis that such outside—of-class activities as those mentioned above dg_affect one‘s scholastic performance and, further, that this effect is a negative one. To accept this as conclusive would be erroneous, for only associations have been explored and to say that a cause and effect relationship exists would be going beyond the justifiable limits of the data presented. Nevertheless, association between two 137 variables always suggests the possibility of one causing the other, and in this case, particiaption in various out—of-class activities naturally leads to questions regarding its impact on scholastic performance. The facts are these: examination of student partici- pation in extra-curricular activities from the standpoint of amount of time required, reveals that negative scholastic performance changers were consistently involved in extra- curricular activities more often than expected. Living in a fraternity or sorority house is also consistently associated with a negative change in scholastic performance patterns, and, though not to the same degree, part—time employment is associated with a downward change. The basic nature of the relationship between the student's outside—of-class activities and in-class performance is challenged by these data. And since the findings seem to contradict the findings reported by other researchers, one cannot help wondering if the techniques employed in this investigation are not worthy of consideration in future research of this kind. Particularly the pattern analysis approach to achievement seems to be promising. 138 In any event, factors such as those examined in this investigation must be examined more thoroughly in terms of their relationship to academic performance. Only a fraction of the variance in academic achievement is accounted for by such standard indices as ability, aptitude, and previous performance, and even the relationships between these variables and scholastic achievement diminishes rapidly after one year in college. In fact, little attention has been given to achievement beyond the first one or two years, and it is here that behavioral variables of the kind used in this investi— gation might prove especially helpful. The steady decrease in relationship between scholastic achievement and measures of ability and aptitude suggests the possibility of other factors contributing to the variance in performance. New demands on the student's time, suoh as part-time employment, or more involvement in organized activities, could well be the factors in question. If this is the case -- and the data resulting from this study seem to give this indication - then a new look at the student out—of-class seems to be in order. Extra-curricular activities, it seems should still be regarded as "extra," and closer examination must be given to the effect they have on the academic performance of those who consider such activity as a major purpose or goal of higher education. 139 The findings of this investigation suggest continued use of behavioral variables as possible factors in scholastic performance, and indicate that the concept of performance change analysis provides a promising methodological technique for such an endeavor. Implications For Further Research An attempt was made in this study to identify factors which seem to be related to changes in the scholastic perform— ance patterns of college students. The variables chosen for this study have by no means eXhausted the possibilities in this area. On the other hand, a number of the variables analyzed in this investigation have been demonstrated to lack association. The results of thiszstudy, then, suggest the following considerations for further research in this area. 1) A more concentrated emphasis on behavioral variables would be a desirable approach, with the further suggestion of an interview technique to ascertain the factors associated with performance changers. 2) The present investigation considered only associ- ations between various factors and performance change. Ideally a study would be designed to 3) 4) 140 isolate the causes of the performance change. This is closely related to the first suggestion, since an interview technique would probably be one of the more promising techniques employed in attempting to ferret out the reasons and causes of academic performance change. A replication of the present study or any approach which has a similar purpose, should make a thorough analysis of such factors as repeated courses, credit- hour load, curriculum, and level of courses taken, and control on these variables in the design of the research. Elimination of the achievement grouping would be desirable. This technique was employed in the present study because of the known relationship between some of the variables analyzed and the level of student achievement. In further research, however, since it is likely that ability and aptitude factors would be omitted, the adhievement grouping would also be a technique to omit. This would considerably reduce the number of groups to be analyzed. and yield easier interpretation to the data. 5) 6) 141 If possible, a larger confidence band should be used in determining changes in scholastic performance. The standard error of estimate chosen will determine, to a great extent, the nature of the performance change groups. Utilization of a larger standard error would result in larger actual performance changes for those being classified in one of the change groups. 1 Continued use of changes in performance patterns as a methodolOgical technique is encouraged, along with such analyses in the students' later terms in school. 10. BIBLIOGRAPHY Aller, Florence D. "Some Factors in Marital Adjustment and Academic Achievement of Married Students," The Personnel and Guidance Journal. XLI (1960), 217-220. Anastasi, Anne. Psychological Testing. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1961. Bach, Mary L. "Factors Related to Student Participation in Campus Social Organizations," Journal of Social Psychology. LIV (1961). 337-348. Bennett, George K., Bennett, Marjorie G., Wallace, Winburn L., and Wesman, Alexander G. College Qualifi- cation Tests, Manual, 1957. New York: The PsycholOgi- cal Corporation, 1957. Bloom, B. S., and Heyns, I. Dev. "Development and Applications of Tests of Educational Achievement," Review of Educational Research. XXVI (1956), 72-88. Buckner, Donald R. "A Comparison of the Scholarship of Freshmen Fraternity Pledges and Residence Hall Men," Journal of College Student Personnel. III (1961), 20-25. Clark, Burton R. "The 'Cooling-Out' Function in Higher Education," American Journal of Sociology, LXV (1960), 569-576. Computer Laboratory, Michigan State University. "KS-M, Correlation, Means, Standard Deviations, Variances: Card Input," MISTIC Library Index. East Lansing, Michigan: 1959. Computer Laboratory, Michigan State University. ”K6-M, Chi-Square for K x L Tables," MISTIC Library Index. East Lansing, Michigan: 1957. Cooperative Study of Evaluation in General Education, Paul L. Dressel, Director. Instructor's Manual for the Inventory of Beliefs. The American Council on Education, Committee on Measurement and Evaluation, 1953 (Mimeo- graphed). 142 ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. l6. 17. 18. 19. 20. 143 Cooperative Study of Evaluation in General Education, Paul L. Dressel, Director. Instructor's Manual for the Test of Critical Thinking, Form G. The American Council on Education,rCommittee on Measurement and Evaluation, 1953 (MimeOgraphed). Crookston, Burns B. “Academic Performance of Fraternity Pledges," The Journal of College Student Personnel. I (1960), 19-22. Dickinson, Carl, and Newbegin, Betty. "Can Work and College Mix," Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXVIII (1959), 314-318. Diener, Charles L. "Similarities and Differences Between Over-Achieving and Under-Achieving Students," The Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXVIII (1960), 396—400. Dressel, Paul L., and Mayhew, Lewis B. General Education: Explorations in Evaluation. washington, D.C.: American Council on Education, 1954. Ehrlich, H. J. "Dogmatism and Learning," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. LXII (1961), 148-149. Erb, Everett D. "Conformity and Achievement in College," Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXIX (1961), 361-368. Farnsworth, Dana S. "Some Non-Academic Causes of Success and Failure in College Students," College Entrance Examination Board, College Admissions, No. 2. Princeton, New Jersey: The Board, 1955, 72-78. Fink, Joseph. "Collecting and Quantifying Social Partici- pation Information," The Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXVI (1958), 417-420. Fisher, Margaret B. "Trends in College Students' Grades," The Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXIX (1961), 491-494. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 144 Fishman, Joshua A. "Non-Intellective Factors as Predictions, as Criteria, and as Contingencies in Selection and Guid— ance of College Students: A Socio—Psychological Analysis," in Conference on Selection and Educational Differentiation. Selection and Educational Differentiation: Proceedings. Berkeley, California: The Center for the Study of Higher Education, 1960, 55-73. Fredericksen, N., and Schrader, W. B. Adjustment to College. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1951. Fredericksen, N., and Schrader, W. B. "The A. C. E. Psychological Examination and High School Standing as Predictors of College Success," Journal of Applied Psychology. XXXIX (1955), 39, 49-52. Freedman, N. B. "The Passage Through College," Journal of Social Issues. XII (1956), 13-28. Freeman, Edward M., and Johnson, Palmer 0. "Prediction of Success in the College of Agriculture, Forestry, and Home Economics," in University of Minnesota Studies in Predicting Scholastic Achievement, Part I. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 1942. Frick, J. W. "Improving the Prediction of Academic Achievement by Use of the M.M.P.I.," Journal of Applied Psychology. XXXIX (1955), 49-52. Fricke, B. G. The Opinion, Attitude and Interest Survey. Minneapolis: Investors Diversified Services, 1955. Garrett, Harley F. "A Review of and Interpretation of Investigations of Factors Related to Scholastic Success in Colleges of Arts and Science and Teachers Colleges," Journal of Experimental Education. XVIII (1949), 91-138. Gough, H. G. "Factors Related to Differential Achievement Among Gifted Persons." Paper read at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, September, 1955. Gough, H. G. "The Construction of a Scale to Predict Scholastic Achievement," Journal of Applied Psyghology. XXXVII (1953). 361-366. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 145 Hansmeier, Thomas W. "The Iowa Tests of Educational Development as Predictors of College Achievement," Educational and Psycholpgical Measurement. XX (1960), 843-847. Harris, P. "The Relation of College Grades to Some Factors Other Than I.Q." Archives of Psychology, XX (1931), 136-141. Heist, Paul A. "Diversity in College Student Character— istics." Journal of Educational Sociology. XXXIII (1960), 217-220. Heist, Paul A. "Variations in the Personality Character- istics of Underachieving College Students." Paper presented at the annual meeting of The American College Personnel Association, Philadelphia, 1960. Heist, Paul A., and Williams, Phoebe, A. "Variation in Achievement Within a Select and Homogeneous Student Body." Unpublished research report. Center for The Study of Higher Education, University of California, 1958. Hodges, Harold M. "Campus Leaders and Non-Leaders," Sociology and Social Research. XXXVII (1953) 251-255. Holland, John L. "The Prediction of College Grades from the California Psychological Inventory and Scholastic Aptitude Test," Journal of Educational Psychology. L (1959), 135-139. Horst, P. "A Technique for the Development of a Differential Prediction Battery," Psychological Monographs. LXIX (1955). Horst, P. "The Differential Prediction of Success in Various College Course Areas," College and Universipy. XXXII (1956), 539-546. Howard, Victor, and Warrington, Willard. "The Inventory of Beliefs: Changes in Beliefs and Attitudes and Academic Success Prediction," Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXVII (1958), 299-302. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47 48. 49 50. 146 Hunter, E. C., and Jordan, A. M. ”An Analysis of Qualities Associated with Leadership Among College Students," Journal of Educational Psychology. XXX (1939), 497-509. Iffert, Robert E. Retention and Withdrawal of College Students, Bulletin No. l, 1958. Office of Education, Washington: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1957. Ikenberry, Stanley 0. "A Multivariate Analysis of the Relationship of Academic Aptitude, Social Background, Attitudes and Values to Collegiate Persistence." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1960. Iowa, State University. A Study of Student Persistence at the State University of Iowa. Iowa City, Office of the Registrar, 1959. Jensen, Vern H., and Clark, Monroe H. “Married and Un- married College Students: Achievement, Ability and Personality," The Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXVII (1958). 123-126. JOhnson, G. B. "A Proposed Technique for the Analysis of Dr0p—Outs at a State College,“ Journal of Educational Research. XLVII (1954), 381-387. Juola, Arvo E. "Freshman-Level Ability Tests and Long- Range Prediction," Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Council on Measurements Used in Education, Chicago, 1963. Juola, Arvo E. "Predictive validity of Five College- Level Academic Aptitude Tests at One Institution," Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXVIII (1960), 637-641. Juola, Arvo E. "The Differential Validity of the College Qualification Tests for Diverse Curricular Groups," Personnel and Guidance Journal. XXXIX (1961), 721-725. Klugh, Henry E., and Bierley, Robert. "The School and College Ability Test and High School Grades as Predictors of College Achievement," Educational and Psychological Measurement. XIX (1959), 625-628. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 147 Knoell, Dorothy. "Institutional Research on Retention and Withdrawal," Research on College Students. Co-sponsored by The Western Insterstate Commission for Higher Education, Boulder, Colorado, and The Center for Higher Education, Berkeley, California. Hall T. Sprague (editor), December, 1960. ' Lantagne, Joseph E. "College Marriages," The Journal of College Student Personnel, III (1962), 98-105. Lehmann, Irvin J. "The RelationShip Between Scholastic Performance and Attitudes and Values," 17th Yearbook, National Council on Measurements Used in Education. Ames, Iowa: National Council on Measurements Used in Education, 1960, 83-92. Lehmann, Irvin J., and Ikenberry, Stanley 0. "Critical Thinking, Attitudes, and Values in Higher Education." A Preliminary Report of Research, Paul L. Dressel, Principle Investigator. Michigan State University, 1959. Lins, L. J. "Probability Approach to Forecasting Uni- versity Success with Measured Grades as Criterion," Educational and Psychological Measurement. X (1950). 386-391. Lins, L. J., and Pitt, Hy. "The 'Staying Power' and Rate of Progress of University of Wisconsin Freshmen," College and Universipy. XXIX (1953), 86-99. Maier, G. E. "The Contribution of Interest Test Scores to Differential Academic Prediction," Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Washington, 1957. McNeeley, John H. College Student morgality. U.S. Department of Interior Bulletin, No. 11, 1937. McNemar, Quinn. Psychological Statistics. 2nd edition. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1955. Morgan, H. H. "A Psychometric Comparison of Achieving and Non-Achieving College Students of High Ability," Journal of Consulting Psychology. XVI (1952), 292-298. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 148 Mueller, Kate H. "The Married Student On The Campus," College and University. XXXV (1960), 155-163. Neel, Ann Filinger. "The Relationship of Authoritarian Personality to Learning: F-scale Scores Compared to Classroom Performance," Journal of Educational Psycholog . L (1959), 195-199. Pattishall, E. G., Jr., and Banghart, F. W. "A Comparative Analysis of School of Education Graduates and‘Withdrawals," Educational Research Bulletin. University of Virginia, April, 1957. Pearlman, S. "An Investigation of the Problem of Academic Underachievement Among Intellectually Superior College Students." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, New York University, 1952. Plant, Walter T., Minium, Edward W., and Myers, Celestine. "An Analysis of the Rokeach Dogmatism Scale Used with a Sample of American College Students." Paper read at the annual meeting of The Western Psychological Association, San Diego, 1959. Prince, Richard. "A Study of the Relationship Between Individual Values and Administrative Effectiveness in the School Situation." Unpublidhed Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, 1957. Red, S. B., McCary, J. L., and Johnson, Bette. "A Study of the Relationship Between Aspirational Levels and Academic Achievement," Journal of Educational Research. LV (1962), 159-163. Rokeach, Milton. "Political and Religious Dogmatism: An Alternative to Authoritarian Personality,“ Psychological Monographs. LXX (1956), 1-10. Rust, R. M., and Ryan, F. J. "Personality and Academic Achievement: A Questionnaire Approach." Paper read at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, 1955. Samenfink, J. Anthony, and Milliken, Robert L. "Marital Status and Academic Success: A Reconsideration," Marriage and Family Living. XXIII (1961), 226-227. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 149 Seashore, Harold G. "Women Are More Predictable Than Men." Journal of Counselipg Psychology. IX (1962), 261-264. Sharp, H. C., and Pickett, L. M. "The General Aptitude Test Battery as a Predictor of College Success," Educational and Psychological Measurement. XIX (1959), 617-620. Spindler, George. "Education in a Transforming American Culture," Harvard Educational Review. XXV (1953), 156-163. Stright, I. L. "Some Factors Affecting College Success," Journal of Educational Psychology. XXXVIII (1947), 232-240. Suddarth, Betty M. "Factors Influencing the Graduation of Freshmen Who Enroll at Purdue University." Unpub- lished report, June, 1957. Summerskill, thn. "Dropouts from College," Chapter 19 in The American College (Nevitt Sanford, Editor). New York: Wiley and Sons, 1962. Thompson, Martha. "Admission Information as Predictors for Graduation." Unpublished Master's Thesis, Cornell University, 1953. Travers, Robert M. "Significant Research on the Prediction of Academic Success," in The Measurement of Student Adjustment and Achievement (Wilma Donahue, Editor). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1949. Trueblood, Dennis L. "Academic Achievement of Employed and Non-Employed Students at The Indiana University of Business," Dissertation Abstracts. XIV (1954), 643-644. Walker, Helen M., and Lev, Joseph. Statistical Inference. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1953. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 150 Weigand, G. "Motivational Factors Associated with Success and Failure of Probational Students." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Maryland, 1951. Williamson, E. G., and Hoyt, D. "Measured Personality Characteristics of Student Leaders," Educational and Psychological Measurement. XII (1952), 65-78. Willingham, Warren W. "College Performance of Fraternity Members and Independent Students," Personnel and Guid- ance Journal. XL (1962), 29-34. Winter, William D. "Student Values and Grades in General Psychology,“ Journal ofAEducational Research. LV (1962), 331-334. Winter, William D. "Values and Achievement in a FreShman PsycholOgy Course," Journal of Educational Research. LIV (1961), 183-185. Wrenn, C. Gilbert. Student Personnel Work in College. New York: The Ronald Press Company, 1961. Young, F. Chandler. Scholastic Progress Forecasts. University of Wisconsin, 1961 (Mimeoqraphed). APPENDIX 151 IT 152 TABLE 6.1: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Test of Critical Thinking. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of variabilipy §§_ 'gf V_ F. 52 Among 22.71 2 11.35 0.25 3.89 Within 10,270.89 224 45.85 Total 10,293.60 226 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability. §§_ HE. E; F ES Among 69.05 2 34.52 0.89 3.04 Within 8,118.87 210 38.66 Total 8,187.92 212 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability §§_ 8E. V_ ‘F ES Among 15.86 2 7.93 0.23 3.04 Within 7,323.47 212 34.54 Total 7,339.33 214 153 TABLE 6.2: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, College Qualification Tests. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of variability SS_ SS y_ .E 'SE Among 1,264.12 2 632.06 1.32 3.89 Within 107,104.12 224 478.15 Total 108,368.90 226 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability .§§ _SS 2 S. So Among 48.54 2 24.27 0.06 3.04 Within 86,691.75 210 412.82 Total 86,740.29 212 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS ‘QS ‘V S ‘SS Among 2,365.52 2 1,182.76 2.74 3.04 Within 91,545.10 212 431.82 Total 93,910.62 214 154 TABLE 6.3: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Michigan State University Reading Test. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS if y_ S £9 Among 62.08 2 31.04 1.44 3.89 Within 4,823.46 224 21.53 Total 4,885.54 226 MIDDLE.ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ {SS Y. .3 So Among 189.85 2 94.92 3.03 3.04 Within 6,562.72 210 31.25 Total 6,752.57 212 Low ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS 1 _d_f_ _\_I_ S ES. Among 138.10 2 69.05 2.50 3.04 Within 5,851.83 212 27.60 Total 5,989.93 214 155 TABLE 7.1: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Test of Critical Thinking. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ .8: X .E .52 Among 292.18 2 146.09 4.46 3.07 Within 4,089.69 125 32.72 Total 4,381.87 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ “SS V S SS Among 37.18 2 18.59 0.57 3.07 Within 4,034.57 124 32.54 Total 4,071.75 126 LOW'ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS. SS ‘2 S SE Among 61.71 2 30.85 0.92 3.07 Within 4,301.57 128 33.61 Total 4,363.28 130 156 TABLE 7.2: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, College Qualification Tests. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ g£_ _! §_ ‘So Among 649.44 2 324.72 0.66 3.07 Within 61,730.28 125 493.84 Total 62,379.72 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ S: V; S ‘So Among 13.51 2 6.75 0.02 3.07 Within 44,919.70 124 362.26 Total 44,933.21 126 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ SE_ 2' E. .E2 Among 950.65 2 475.32 1.39 3.07 Within 43,810.88 128 342.27 Total 44,761.53 130 157 TABLE 7.3: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Michigan State University Reading Test. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS g; ‘2' S .SE Among 69.97 2 34.98 2.00 3.07 Within 2,183.58 125 17.47 Total 2,253.55 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS. SS ‘1 §_ .52 Among 103.12 2 51.56 2.46 3.07 Within 2,596.96 124 20.94 Total 2,700.08 126 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of variability SS_ SS .2 ,E SE Among 46.15 2 23.07 1.07 3.07 Within ‘ 2,753.90 128 21.51 Total 2,800.05 130 158 TABLE 8.1: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Inventory of Beliefs. HIGH AcngVEMENT Component of variability SS_ 1g: 2 ‘S ES Among 647.09 2 323.54 1.82 3.89 Within 39,882.43 224 178.05 Total 40,529.52 226 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ SS .2 ‘S ‘SE Among 317.14 2 158.57 0.81 3.04 Within 41,250.59 210 196.43 Total 41,567.73 212 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component_9f variability_ SS_ ‘SS .2 'S SE Among 337.63 2 168.81 0.83 3.04 Within 43,341.55 212 204.44 Total 43,679.18 214 159 TABLE 8.2: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Differential Values Inventory. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of variability. SS_ ‘SS y_ 'S SE Among 201.29 2 100.64 1.99 3.89 Within 11,314.43 224 50.51 Total 11,515.72 226 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT_ Component of Variability SS 9}: X S So Among 30.34 2 15.17 0.35 3.04 Within 9,226.94 210 43.94 Total 9,257.28 212 LOW ACHIEVEMENS Component of variability» SS .85 y: ‘S Fc Among 113.39 2 56.69 1.29 3.04 Within 9,345.87 212 44.08 Total 9,459.26 214 160 TABLE 8.3: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on fall, 1958, Dogmatism Scale. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ SS .2 S .ES Among 1,609.50 2 804.75 1.37 3.89 Within 131,609.50 224 585.78 Total 132,825.07 226 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS. SS .2 .5 SS Among 1,023.98 2 511.99 0.80 3.04 Within 135,119.01 210 643.42 Total 136,142.99 212 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of variability SS_ ‘SS 1!» S SS Among 796.73 2 398.36 0.60 3.04 Within 139,635.20 212 658.66 Total 140,431.93 214 161 TABLE 9.1: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on spring, Inventory of Beliefs. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability Among 451. Within 150.282. Total . 150.734. MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability Among 3.802. Within 100,555. Total 104,358. LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability Among 2,181. Within 98.078. Total 100,259. 99. 28 80 08 9.8 71 58 29 .85 31 67 98 2 212 214 31 225.64 670.91 1’. 1901.35 478.84 1’. L090.65 462.64 IW 0.34 IW 2.36 1959. 3.89 3.04 162 TABLE 9.2: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement males on spring, 1959, Differential Values Inventory. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS. SS. .2 S SS Among 128.79 2 64.39 0.39 3.89 Within 37,044.76 224 165.38 Total 37,173.55 226 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ ‘SS 1! S SS Among 538.11 2 269205 2.22 3.04 Within 25,401.55 210 120.96 Total 25,939.66 212 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS ‘SS S S. SS Among 97.88 2 48.94 0.37 3.04 Within 27,714.75 212 130.73 Total 27,812.63 214 163 TABLE 10.1: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Inventory of Beliefs. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ SS_ .2 S .52 Among 43.60 2 21.80 0.16 3.07 Within 17,283.64 125 138.27 Total 17,327.24 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ ‘SS .2 S SS Among 190.68 2 95.34 0.85 3.07 Within 14,982.58 124 112.53 Total 15,173.26 126 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ .8: ‘S S SS Among 15.49 2 7.74 0.04 3.07 Within 23,573.41 125 184.17 Total 23,588.90 127 TABLE 10.2: 164 Differential Values Inventory. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability Among 69. Within 6,352. Total 6,421. MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability Among 23. Within 5,661. Total 5,685. LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability Among 133. Within 6.484. Total 6,618. 9.9. 47 41 88 99. 51 67 18 .89 43 62 05 l< 34.73 50.82 |< 11.75 45.66 |< 66.71 50.66 Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958. 2:: Es 0.68 3.07 11 31c. 0.26 3.07 S_ Fc 1.32 3.07 165 TABLE 10.3: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on fall, 1958, Dogmatism Scale. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ .SS ‘2 S_ .32 Among 243.96 2 121.98 0.26 3.07 Within 59,001.34 125 472.01 Total 59,245.30 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ SS S .E. (ES Among 1,279.03 2 639.51 1.06 3.07 Within 74,950.84 124 604.44 Total 75,229.87 126 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS ‘SS ‘2’ S SS Among 479.73 2 239.86 0.36 3.07 Within 84,732.80 128 661.97 Total ~85,212.53 130 166 TABLE 11.1: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on spring. 1959. Inventory of Beliefs. HIGH ACHIEVEMSNT Component of Variability SS .SS .2 S. SS Among 838.70 2 419.35 0.99 3.07 Within 52,847.52 125 422.78 Total 53,686.22 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ ‘SS ‘S S SS Among 1,239.43 2 619.71 1.74 3.07 Within 44,089.63 124 355.56 Total 45,329.06 126 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS. ‘SS 2; S SS Among 1,218.96 2 609.50 1.17 3.07 Within 66,484.58 128 519.41 Total 67,703.54 130 167 TABLE 11.2: Analysis of variance data for high, middle, and low achievement females on spring, 1959. Differential Values Inventory. HIGH ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ 'SS ‘2 ‘S SS Among 163.20 2 81.60 0.96 3.07 Within 10,591.42 125 84.73 Total 10,754.62 127 MIDDLE ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability ‘SS 'SS 2: S_ SS ' Among 435.27 2 217.63 2.35 3.07 Within 11,492.60 124 92.68 Total 11,927.87 126 LOW ACHIEVEMENT Component of Variability SS_ SS_ 2. ‘S SS Among 202.63 2 101.31 0.73 3.07 Within 17,766.12 128 138.80 Total 17,968.75 130 P’ 'H' O : C' r ‘ J'Sf‘ .’ 394“," I .5" 1‘. 2 3 ‘5 (1' a r ' 1' f ' ' - L H. , ‘ l\ d “j“, ‘ 9 . ‘ J .r.. . II. t w. I“. ...‘ m . . 1‘. ERSI WTITIIIWINWTIWITUTW WI! W "111111111111“ 3 1293 0301] 9743