INVESTIGATIONS DIsseItatIon for the Degree at Ph D MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY STEVEN G BRANDL 1991 ’3‘. THE ourcows AND PROCESSES OE DETECTIVE”-fiig-‘i’fjllik:5 " ,jiDEClSION MAKING IN BURGLARY AND ROBBERY I03 058 TN 6 .UBRARY H. ICE”I§gE§Z.-II State nIve.'in'y I fi (c. PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5m KIProVAcc5Pras/CIRCIDateDuo.Nd THE OUTCOMES AND PROCESSES OF DETECTIVE DECISION MAKING IN BURGLARY AND ROBBERY INVESTIGATIONS by Steven G. Brandl A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY School of Criminal Justice College of Social Science 1991 ABSTRACT THE OUTCOMES AND PROCESSES OF DETECTIVE DECISION MAKING IN BURGLARY AND ROBBERY INVESTIGATIONS By Steven G. Brandl This study described the cognitive processes associated with detective decision making and examined the influence of case (victim and offense) characteristics on detectives’ decisions to (1) select a case for a follow-up investigation and (2) allocate varying amounts of time to a follow-up investigation. The data were gathered from a medium sized Midwestern police department. Three methodologies were used. First, case characteristics were coded from burglary (N = 857) and robbery (N = 305) investigative reports and the resulting data were used in OLS regression analyses to determine the relationship between case characteristics and decision outcomes. Second, an information board was used to collect verbal protocol data from burglary and robbery detectives (N = 10). These data provided insight into the depth, content, and linearity of search. Third, observations (370 hours) of detectives allowed for additional insight into the factors which influence decisions and the cognitive processes associated with decision making. The OLS multiple regression analyses showed that victim age, race, sex, income, employment status, and identifiability of the stolen property did not affect decision making. Dollar value of the stolen property, strength of suspect information, and presence of physical evidence did have a significant impact on decision making. Victim type, victim desire for effort, victim- offender relationship, presence of a suspect vehicle description and license number displayed inconsistent effects across decisions. Observations and verbal protocol analyses showed interaction and dependency effects among many of the variables and illustrated the extensive use of linear decision making strategies by investigators. These findings are discussed in relation to their theoretical contribution to the detective, police, and criminal Justice decision making literature. To Kara who has made countless sacrifices and provides so much support iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I am indebted to many people. I would like to thank the administration of the Landau Police Department which allowed me access to their organization and to Dr. Robert Trojanowicz whose support was instrumental in gaining entry to the department. Several faculty members -- Drs. Robert Worden, Peter Manning, Micheal Lindell, Kevin Ford, and Chairman Frank Horvath -- all helped shape and improve this dissertation. My thanks also go to Jim Frank who assisted me on this and many other projects over the past several years. Finally, I would like to thank the subjects of this study -- the detectives of the Landau Police Department. They expanded my reality by letting me study theirs. For this I am deeply grateful. iv TABLE OF LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION . . Problem . . . . . . . . . . . Purpose of the Study . . . . CONTENTS Page 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 4 A Definitional Model of the Investigative Process . . . 5 Initial Discovery and Notification . . . . . . . . . 7 Initial Investigation . . . Follow-up Investigation . . Closure . . . . . . ... . . Definition of Terms . . . . . Overview of Study . . . . . . Footnotes . . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER TWO LITERATURE REVIEW 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 7 I O I O O 0 O O O O O O O 13 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 0 14 The Decision Tasks of Detectives . . . . . . . . . . . 14 Case Selection Decision . . Time Allocation Decision . Analytical Foundations for Res Decisions as Outcomes . . . Victim Characteristics and 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 15 O O O O O O O O O O O O O 19 earch on Decision Making 23 O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O 25 Decision Making . . . . 26 Offense Characteristics and Decision Making . . . . 36 V Decisions as Processes Summary . . . . . . . . . Footnotes . . . . . . . . CHAPTER THREE RESEARCH SITE . . . . . . . The City 0 o o o o o o o The Landau Police Department . . . . . . The Investigative Process Summary . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER FOUR METHODOLOGY Decisions as Outcomes . . Case Selection Procedure Variables . . . . . . . Analysis . . . . . . . Hypotheses . . . . . . Decisions as Processes . The Information Board / Subjects . . . . . . . Information Board Structure and Content Procedure . . . . . . Research Questions . . Analysis . . . . . . . The Observation Method Footnotes . . . . . . . . CHAPTER FIVE RESULTS . . Decisions as Outcomes . . at the Landau P. Verbal Protocol vi Analysis page 41 48 49 50 5O 51 55 62 63 64 66 67 74 75 77 77 77 78 80 82 83 85 9O 91 91 Page The Selection of Burglaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 Time Allocation in Burglary Investigations . . . . . 100 Time Allocation in Robbery Investigations . . . . . . 109 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 Decisions as Processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 The Selection of Burglaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120 Prioritization of Burglaries . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 Prioritization of Robberies . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142 Observations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 Footnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 154 CHAPTER SIX DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 Decision Outcomes in Context . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 Case Characteristics and Decision Outcomes . . . . . . 158 The Processes of Decision Making . . . . . . . . . . . 174 Limitations of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 178 Directions for Future Research . . . . . . . . . . . . 183 Footnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186 APPENDIX A - Initial Investigation Report Form . . . . . 187 APPENDIX B - Supplemental Report Form . . . . . . . . . 189 APPENDIX C - Modus Operandi Descriptor Form . . . . . . 190 APPENDIX D - Personal Descriptor Form . . . . . . . . . 191 APPENDIX E - Vehicle Descriptor Form . . . . . . . . . . 192 APPENDIX F - Property Form . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 193 vii page APPENDIX C - Follow-up Investigation Case Log . . . . . 194 APPENDIX H - Case Data Coding Form . . . . . . . . . . . 195 APPENDIX I - Information Board: Selection of Burglaries 196 APPENDIX J - Information Board: Prioritization of Burglaries O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O 198 APPENDIX K - Information Board: Prioritization of Robberies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 200 APPENDIX L - Information Board Instructions for the Selection of Burglaries . . . . . . . . . 202 APPENDIX M - Depth of Search Formula and Examples . . . 203 APPENDIX N - Content of Search Formula and Examples . . 204 APPENDIX 0 - Linearity of Search Formula and Examples . 205 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 207 viii TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE 10 11 12 LIST OF TABLES Independent and Dependent Variables: Values and Descriptive Statistics by all Burglaries O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Correlation Coefficients Among Variables for all Burglaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . Probit (and Multiple Regression) Analysis of Burglary Case Selection as a Function of Victim and Offense Characteristics . . . . . Independent and Dependent Variables: Values and Descriptive Statistics by "Selected" Burglaries O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Frequency of Detective Activities in Burglary FOIIOW'UP Investigations 0 o o o o e o o e 0 Correlation Coefficients Among Variables for "Selected" Burglaries . . . . . . . . . . Multiple Regression of Time Spent on Burglary Follow-up Investigations as a Function of Victim and Offense Characteristics . . . . . Independent and Dependent Variables: Values and Descriptive Statistics by "Selected" RObberieB O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Frequency of Detective Activities in Robbery Follow-up Investigations . . . . . . . . . . Correlation Coefficients Among Variables for "Selected" Robberies . . . . . . . . . . Multiple Regression of Time Spent on Robbery Follow-up Investigations as a Function of Victim and Offense Characteristics . . . . . Proportion of Available Information Searched in the Burglary Selection Decision . . . . . ix Page 92 95 97 101 103 105 107 110 113 114 117 121 TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE TABLE 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 Page The Importance of Case Information in the Burglary Selection Decision . . . . . . . . . 122 Linearity of Search in the Burglary Selection Decision . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 Proportion of Available Information Searched in the Prioritization of Burglary Cases . . . 128 The Importance of Case Information in the Prioritization of Burglary Cases . . . . . . 130 Linearity of Search in the Prioritization of Burglary Cases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 Proportion of Available Information Searched in the Prioritization of Robbery Cases . . . 137 The Importance of Case Information in the Prioritization of Robbery Cases . . . . . . . 138 Linearity of Search in the Prioritization of Robbery Cases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 Decision Outcomes for Burglary and Robbery Cases in Eck (1983) and Present Study . . . . 157 LIST OF FIGURES Page FIGURE 1 Landau Police Department Organizational Chart 0 I O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 52 FIGURE 2 A Decision Making Model of Burglary and Robbery Investigations . . . . . . . . . . . 144 xi CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION Chapter One contains an introduction to the research. The problem and purpose of the study are discussed and the definitions used in the study are presented. Chapter One concludes with an overview of the dissertation. Problem The criminal justice system, that mechanism of society created to deal with crime and criminals, can be conceptualized as a sequential series of decision stages. Research attention has been directed at examining the critical decisions of participants within each of the stages. For example, the victim’s decision to report a crime has been analyzed (Hindelang & Gottfredson, 1976; Laub, 1981), along with the police decision to arrest (Black, 1971; Smith & Visher, 1981; Visher, 1983; Worden & Pollitz, 1984; Smith, 1987) and investigate (Bynum, Cordner, & Greene, 1982), judicial decision to grant pretrial release (Frazier, Bock, & Henretta, 1980; Nagel, 1983), prosecutor decision to charge (Adams & Cutshall, 1987; Albonetti, 1986; Schmidt & Steury, 1989), and plea bargain (Holmes, Duadistel, & Farrell, 1987), juridic decision to convict (Brooks & Doob, 1975), judicial decision to sentence (Baldus, Pulanski, & Woodworth, 1983; Platt-Jendrek, 1984; Welch & Spohn, 1986), and parole board decision to grant release (Von Hirsch & Hanrahan, 1979). While most of these stages and participants have been the objects of extensive research attention, little research has focused specifically on investigative decisions by detectives. In fact, only one study in the literature has taken this as its primary focus (Bynum, Cordner, & Greene, 1982). Other studies with a broader focus on the investigative process (e.g., Eck, 1983; Greenwood, Chaiken & Petersilia, 1977; Sanders, 1977) have contributed only indirectly to our understanding of detective decision making. There appear to be at least two reasons for the lack of research on this topic. First, unlike many other decision stages in the justice process (e.g., arrest, plea bargain, convict, sentence) the decision to investigate is characterized by a relatively low degree of visibility (Ericson, 1981). The decision occurs ”backstage” and therefore, is not often open to public scrutiny. As a result, the topic may be simply overlooked by researchers. Second, detective decision making has been widely portrayed as being "routine" (Eck, 1983; Greenwood et al., 1977; Sanders, 1977) where the strength of the evidence is assumed to automatically determine the disposition of the case. Given this widely shared (but untested) reality, few researchers have deemed this topic as particularly worthy of research attention. The lack of research on detective decision making is troubling. A substantial amount of police resources and activities are allocated to follow-up investigations yet we know very little about this decision stage. From the perspective of developing an adequate understanding of police decision making in total, this is inherently unsettling. Relatedly, students of investigative management have long called for strategies to increase the capacity of the police to apprehend offenders. However, a prerequisite for improving the effectiveness of the criminal investigation process is a sound understanding of the process. As Ericson (1981) explains, "perhaps most of the proposals for reform have little impact because reformers know too little about what it is they are trying to reform” (p. x). One dimension of a more complete understanding of the investigative process is the identification of the premises and cognitive processes associated with detective decision making. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to analyze detective decision making. To do so, two broad research questions are addressed. First, what case (victim and offense) characteristics influence the decisions to (1) select a case for a follow-up investigation and (2) allocate varying amounts of time to a follow-up investigation? As research has illustrated, the selection of a case for a follow-up investigation does not necessarily mean that attention is given to the case (Bynum et al., 1982; Greenwood et al., 1977; Sanders, 1977). Thus, to provide a thorough inquiry, both decisions within the follow-up investigation are examined. Second, hp! do detectives treat case information in making decisions? Whereas the first question is most concerned with specifying the relationship between the input (information) and the outcome of the decision process (the decision), the second question is concerned primarily with describing the cognitive nggggggg involved in decision making. As such, the two questions emerge from different theoretical perspectives on decision making and require the use of different methodological approaches in order to be addressed. By studying the decision behavior of detectives through the "outcome” and "process" perspectives, it is possible to attain a better understanding of investigative decision making and ultimately, the complexities of the criminal investigation process. ' 4 1 A Definitional Model of the Investigative Process The municipal police organization provides three valued outputs -- service, order maintenance, and law enforcement (i.e. "crime control”) (Wilson, 1968). These outputs also comprise the major categories of work activities within the police organization. "Service” refers to the provision of assistance to the public in regard to non-crime related matters. ”Order maintenance” involves activities oriented" around maintaining the public peace. "Crime control" activities involve intervening in situations where a law has been violated and the identity of the perpetrator needs to be determined. Conceptually, the criminal investigation process can be placed within the crime control aspect of the police mission. Typically, criminal investigations are of a ”reactive" nature, where the police respond to the report of a criminal offense. Some investigations however, especially those associated with vice offenses, are proactive or police initiated (see Manning, 1980; Wilson, 1978). The focus of this study is on the more typical "reactive" type investigation. At the simplest level, the criminal investigation process involves activities oriented around the collection of crime related information in order to: (1) determine if a crime has been committed; (2) identify the perpetrator(s); (3) apprehend the perpetrator(s); and (4) provide evidence to support a conviction in court (Eck, 1983; Greenwood et al., 1977; Kuykendall, 1982). With the arrest rate as the primary measure of investigative effectiveness, arresting offenders (attaining the second and third objective above) has been most often portrayed as the overriding concern of investigators (Greenwood et al., 1977; Waegel, 1981). According to Willmer (1970), the criminal investigation process can be viewed as a battle between the police and the perpetrator over crime related information. That is, the perpetrator, in committing a crime, emits signals (information) which the police attempt to collect through investigative activities. If the perpetrator is able.to minimize the amount of information available for the police to collect, or if the police are unable to recognize the information left behind, then the perpetrator will not be apprehended and therefore, the perpetrator wins the battle. Conversely, if the police are able to collect a significant amount of signals from the perpetrator, then the perpetrator will be apprehended and the police win. For definitional purposes, the (reactive) criminal investigation process can be organized into several stages: initial discovery and response, preliminary investigation, follow-up investigation, and closure. Each of these stages is discussed below. Initial Discovery and Notification In order for the criminal investigation process to be invoked, the police must discover that a crime has taken place and then notify the victim, or the victim (or witness) must discover that a crime has occurred and notify the police. In the vast majority of cases it is the victim who first discovers that a crime occurred and who contacts the police (Greenwood et al., 1977). Then, in most cases, a patrol officer is dispatched to the crime scene. Initial Investigation If, upon arrival, the officer actually defines the matter as a crime (see Black, 1971), then an initial (or preliminary) investigation is conducted. The initial investigation consists of the immediate post-crime activities of the patrol officer who arrives at the crime scene. The officer may proceed to gather information (”signals") concerning the crime by questioning the victim and/or witness(es), searching the crime scene, etc. The specific activities engaged in by the officer may be a function of the particular case at hand. All of the information relating to the crime would then be recorded in an initial investigation report. Follow-up Investigation If a perpetrator is not arrested during the initial investigation, the case may be selected for a follow-up investigation -- the second stage whereby "signals" may be collected. Typically, detective supervisors take the initial investigation reports from the case pool which appear relevant to their unit (e.g., "Homicide," ”Crimes Against Persons,” etc.) and then decide which of the cases should receive a follow-up investigation. If a case is selected for a follow-up, then the detective assigned to the case must decide what activities to perform in the investigation. Depending on the particular case, the follow-up investigation may involve a variety of activities ranging from recontacting and re-interviewing the victim, to submitting evidence to the crime laboratory, to seeking out informants (Eck, 1983). The information which is cultivated as a result of these activities would be recorded in a follow-up report. It is the complexities of the follow-up investigation as well as the case transition process, from the initial investigation to the follow-up investigation, that is of direct interest in this study. Closure At any time during the investigative process the case may be closed and investigative activities terminated. For instance, the case could be closed due to a lack of leads or as a result of the offender being apprehended. In the latter situation, the crime would be considered ”cleared by an arrest” and primary responsibility for the case would shift from the police department to the prosecutor’s office. However, the detective(s) assigned to the case would still have the responsibility of assisting the prosecutor in preparing the case for prosecution. Definition of Terms The following are definitions of terms used in this study. Crime: A crime is the commission of an act prohibited by criminal law or the failure to act as required by criminal law for which punishment is prescribed (Reid, 1989). Specifically, the present study focuses on the crimes of burglary and robbery. As defined by the F.B.I. Uniform Crime Report: "Burglary” refers to the unlawful entry of a structure to commit a felony or theft. The use of force to gain entry is not required to classify an offense as a burglary. (For purposes of this study attempted burglary is not included.) "Robbery" refers to taking (or the attempt to take) anything of value from the care, custody, or control of a person or persons by force or threat of force or violence and/or by putting the victim in fear. Detective: A detective holds a specialized position within the police organization being concerned primarily with the "law enforcement" function of the police mission. Typically, a detective becomes involved in a criminal investigation only after the initial investigation has been completed by a patrol officer. Normally, a detective has the sole responsibility of conducting a follow-up investigation. Detective Sergeant: A detective sergeant is the first line supervisor of detectives within an investigative unit. Detective sergeants have the primary task of deciding which initial investigation reports to assign to detectives for follow-up investigations. Follow-up investigation report: Follow-up reports are produced by detectives and identify the information cultivated as a result of follow-up investigation activities. Investigator: For purposes of this study, an investigator refers to either a detective or a detective sergeant. Official complainant records: Official complainant records are reports completed by patrol officers which detail the nature of the police-citizen contact. In criminal incidents, these reports contain information on the victim and the offense which is obtained through the initial investigation activities. These reports are also known as initial investigation reports. Complainant records are maintained within the police department. Patrol Officer: A patrol officer has broad and diverse responsibilities within the municipal police organization. A patrol officer is concerned with the order maintenance, 10 service, and law enforcement functions of the police mission. In the case of a criminal incident, a patrol officer typically responds to the scene of the crime (and/or the location of the victim) and is responsible for conducting the initial investigation. Personal crime: A personal crime involves the victim directly -- the crime is an attack on the individual. If the crime is directed toward an individual who is a representative of a business establishment, then the crime would be considered a crime against a business. The personal crime of interest in this study is robbery. Property crime: A property crime is directed toward a victim’s property and hence, is an indirect attack on the individual. Again, the property of a business establishment may be the focus of the crime and, in such an instance, the crime would be considered a crime against a business. Burglary is the property crime of interest in this study. Victim: For purposes of this study, a victim is an individual (either a representative of a business or not) that is the object of a criminal act (burglary or robbery) and suffers injuries and/or material losses as a result of the act. Overview of the Study In Chapter Two, the decision tasks of detectives are discussed, the analytical foundations for research on decision making are outlined, and previous research is 11 reviewed. In Chapter Three the research site is described. In Chapter Four, the methodologies used in this study are outlined. The results of the study are presented in Chapter Five. Chapter Six contains the discussion and conclusions. 12 Footnotes This discussion represents a general and generic definitional overview of the criminal investigation process. The mechanics of the investigative process, as found in the present study site, are discussed in Chapter Three. 13 CHAPTER TWO LITERATURE REVIEW Chapter Two begins by discussing the decision tasks of detectives. Attention then turns to the analytical approaches used in studying decision making. Studies which have adopted these approaches in analyzing decisions are reviewed. Through the review, the propositions and research questions addressed in the study are developed. The Decision Tasks of Detectives Myriad studies have highlighted the discretionary nature of police work. Discretion, in this context, refers to ”autonomy of decision making” (Black, 1968, p. 25). As stated by Davis (1969), "a public officer has discretion whenever the effective limits on his power leave him free to make a choice among possible courses of action or_inaction” (p. 4). Simply put, discretion exists when one has the freedom and authority to make decisions. A decision, at the most basic level, is a choice among alternatives based on 14 information and guided by preferences. Just as the criminal justice system can be conceptualized as a sequential series of discretionary decisions, so too can the criminal investigation process. While a model of the criminal investigation process was presented earlier (Chapter One), the purpose of the following discussion is to describe the two critical decision tasks of investigators which correspond to the two stages of the follow-up investigation process: the case selection decision and the ”time allocation” decision. Case Selection Decision Case selection, or screening, the first stage of the follow-up investigation process, typically involves a detective sergeant deciding whether or not the initial investigation report should be assigned to a detective for a follow-up investigation. Depending on the department (or the decision maker) the case screening decision may reflect an aided-analytic strategy, an unaided-analytic strategy, or a non-analytic strategy (Beach and Mitchell, 1978). It is also possible that in some departments, the screening decision does not technically exist. According to Beach and Mitchell (1978), an aided-analytic strategy requires the decision maker "to apply a prescribed procedure utilizing tools such as pencil and paper, mathematics, calculator, or computer, etc. in a 15 guided, systematic attempt to analyze the decision and evaluate its components" (p. 441). Specifically, if a "screening decision model" is used, the decision may resemble the aided analytic strategy. As defined by Eck (1983), the use of a screening decision model... involves simply making a decision to assign or not to assign investigative resources to cases by applying a fixed set of criteria to information cantained in preliminary investigative reports (p. 274). Gaines, Lewis, and Swanagin (1983) add... A case screening process identifies those cases which have the potential for being solved and allows investigators to spend more time on them by eliminating from officers’ caseloads cases which probably cannot be solved due to absence of substantive evidence (p. 22). If an investigative unit used a case screening model, each initial investigation report would be examined in light of the case screening assessment criteria (”solvability factors") and then the utility of a follow-up investigation would be mathematically determined. For example, in the decision model presented by Eek (1983), various information elements (e.g., presence of suspect identification, fingerprints, etc.) are combined in a weighted sum and those cases with a score higher than a certain predetermined score are selected for a follow-up investigation. While the advantages of a case screening system have been noted (Hastings, 1980), there is often much resistance given to the formal use of this type of device in investigative decision making (Eck, 1983). As a result, some departments have instituted policies which identify 16 certain solvability factors to sensitize decision makers to the information on which the screening decision should be based; but weights are not assigned to these elements. As such, the decision to select a case for a follow-up investigation often resembles an "unaided-analytic strategy." With an unaided-analytic strategy, "an attempt is made to explore the dimensions of the problem but... no tools are used, and the decision maker restricts processing to the confines of his or her mind” (Beach & Mitchell, 1978, p. 441). They continue, "unaided analytic strategies have the advantage of reducing information processing by restricting attention to only part of the available information about the alternatives, but they have the disadvantage of introducing possible irrationalities" (p. 442). The screening decision may also reflect a app-analytic strategy where "little information is procured or processed, little time is needed, and the rules do not require that the decision be decomposed nor that its multiple aspects be considered" (p. 442). Examples include flipping a coin or such conventions as "eeny, meeny, miney, mo..." Decisions made by habit, an ”extreme example of rote application of a ' (p. 442) are also non-analytic in nature. rule,’ When the screening decision does not technically exist in a department, all initial investigation reports are given to the detectives and the detectives determine not necessarily which ones to select for an investigation, but 17 which cases should receive the most attention. This is a subtle distinction in practice but one important for analytical purposes. The literature on detective decision making has tended to consider the follow-up investigation process as a whole and as a result, the selection decision as a distinct stage in the process has not received much attention. When the screening decision does receive comment, it is usually only in passing. For example, Sanders (1977) notes, "the sergeant would give one of us researchers the batch of reports to go over and determine which ones would be worked and which ones would not. The selection process was so routine that we rarely made mistakes” (p. 77). No other discussions in the text are devoted to the selection decision. Even Bynum et al. (1982), in an empirical study which focused specifically on detective decision making, did not discuss the screening decision. There exists at least two reasons for this lack of attention. First, the structure and organization of the investigative process in the departments previously studied may not have provided for a "screening decision." Or, second, the decision may have been simply ignored, overshadowed by the other decision stage within the follow-up investigation -- the "decision” as to how much time to devote to an investigation. It is to this stage of the investigative process that attention now turns. 18 certain solvability factors to sensitize decision makers to the information on which the screening decision should be based; but weights are not assigned to these elements. As such, the decision to select a case for a follow-up investigation often resembles an "unaided-analytic strategy." With an unaided-analytic strategy, "an attempt is made to explore the dimensions of the problem but... no tools are used, and the decision maker restricts processing to the confines of his or her mind" (Beach & Mitchell, 1978, p. 441). They continue, "unaided analytic strategies have the advantage of reducing information processing by restricting attention to only part of the available information about the alternatives, but they have the disadvantage of introducing possible irrationalities" (p. 442). The screening decision may also reflect a gen-analytic strategy where "little information is procured or processed, little time is needed, and the rules do not require that the decision be decomposed nor that its multiple aspects be considered" (p. 442). Examples include flipping a coin or such conventions as "eeny, meeny, miney, mo..." Decisions made by habit, an ”extreme example of rote application of a " (p. 442) are also non-analytic in nature. rule, When the screening decision does not technically exist in a department, all initial investigation reports are given to the detectives and the detectives determine not necessarily which ones to select for an investigation, but 17 which cases should receive the most attention. This is a subtle distinction in practice but one important for analytical purposes. The literature on detective decision making has tended to consider the follow-up investigation process as a whole and as a result, the selection decision as a distinct stage in the process has not received much attention. When the screening decision does receive comment, it is usually only in passing. For example, Sanders (1977) notes, "the sergeant would give one of us researchers the batch of reports to go over and determine which ones would be worked and which ones would not. The selection process was so routine that we rarely made mistakes" (p. 77). No other discussions in the text are devoted to the selection decision. Even Bynum et al. (1982), in an empirical study which focused specifically on detective decision making, did not discuss the screening decision. There exists at least two reasons for this lack of attention. First, the structure and organization of the investigative process in the departments previously studied may not have provided for.a "screening decision.” Or, second, the decision may have been simply ignored, overshadowed by the other decision stage within the follow-up investigation -- the "decision" as to how much time to devote to an investigation. It is to this stage of the investigative process that attention now turns. 18 Time Allocation Decision To be accurate, the decision concerning how much time to spend on a given follow-up investigation is not really a single decision, as is the screening decision, but rather a series of interrelated decisions. That is, upon receiving and reviewing an initial investigation report, a detective does not decide how much time to spend on the investigation. Rather, after receiving the report, the detective may (or may not) decide to contact the victim, then he may (or may not) decide to query departmental records, then he may (or may not) decide to interview witnesses, etc. It is only after the detective decides to discontinue any further investigative activities that one can identify the total amount of time the detective spent on the investigation. With a slightly broader perspective, one form of decision making at this stage is case prioritization (Eck, 1979; Ericson, 1981). Detectives are likely to be more willing to spend time on certain cases than on others. Conceptually, case prioritization represents the aggregate outcome of many decisions, decisions which are likely to be at least partially based on victim and offense» -I~ characteristics. The selection decision is relatively well defined and structured with much (but perhaps not all) of the information considered in making the decision likely to be contained within the initial investigatiou"report*(Eek, 1983). On the other hand, decisions concerning the amount 19 of time to devote to a case may be based on not only information contained within the initial investigation report but also on other information gathered directly by the detective through investigative activities. While the search for information in a screening decision could be completed within a matter of minutes, the search and collection of information in the "time allocation decision" could take place over a period of hours, days, or even weeks, thus increasing the complexity of this stage of the follow-up investigation. Previous research on detective decision making has focused most directly on this decision stage. In the previous studies, the time spent on a follow-up investigation, or investigative effort, has been measured in several different ways. For example, Bynum et al. (1982), in an analysis of detective decision making in a ”medium sized midwestern police department," examined the extent to which follow-up investigations were conducted on a sample of 1,124 personal and property crimes reported during a five week time period in 1978 and 1979. The "extent of follow-up investigation" variable had the values of (1) reviewing report only; (2) making a few phone calls; and (3) conducting a more extensive investigation including examining the crime scene, searching for additional witnesses, interviewing suspects, etc. Data on the variable were obtained from a review of follow-up investigation reports completed by detectives. In describing 20 investigative effort, the researchers found that "82 percent of all cases that come to detective attention receive little or no investigative effort" (i.e., fall into categories one and two; p. 315). Eck (1983), in his analysis of preliminary and follow-up investigations and their relative impact on the solution of burglaries and robberies, collected data on 320 robberies and 3,360 burglaries from three police departments -- DeKalb County (GA), St. Petersburg (FL), and Wichita (KS). These departments ranged in size from 374 officers to 445 officers. Data on the time spent by detectives on case investigations came from "activity-time logs" completed by detectives for every case worked. Three measures of investigative time were used: (1) the number of days the case remained open; (2) the number of days on which the case was worked; and (3) the number of minutes actually spent investigating a case. In regard to the last measure of time, Eck found that a mean of 167 minutes (2.8 hours) were spent on robbery follow-up investigations across all study sites and 77 minutes (1.3 hours) were spent on burglary investigations. In Ericson’s (1981) qualitative (observation) study of detective work in ”a jurisdiction in the Canadian province of Ontario" (p. 24), it was found that approximately 30 percent of all cases that came to the attention of the detective bureau received one or more hours of investigative time. In this study, time spent on investigations was 21 estimated on the basis of detailed field observations. According to the study by Greenwood et al. (1977), less than half of all crimes reported to the Kansas City (MO) Police Department "received serious consideration by an investigator” (p. 109). Specifically, based on their review of the department’s "computer-readable case assignment file,” it was found that 63 percent of robberies, 36 percent of non-residential burglaries, and 30 percent of residential burglaries received "at least half an hour of a detective’s time” (p. 130). In regard to how investigative time was actually spent, Ericson (1981) noted that detectives interviewed one or more victims, complainants, and/or informants in 31.5 percent of the cases and suspects in 27.5 percent of the cases. Eck (1983) found that as investigations progressed, they became less routine. The activities performed later in the investigation were more uncommon than those performed earlier in the investigation. For example, the frequency with which victim interviews and crime scene checks were conducted declined over each investigative day while the frequency of suspect interviews increased. With a basic understanding of the decision stages within the follow-up investigation process, attention turns to a discussion of the approaches used to study decision making and a review of the related literature. 22 Analytical Foundations for Research on Decision Making Studies of human decision making have taken one of two analytic approaches (Hogarth, 1974; Payne, 1976). In the first approach, referred to here as "outcome oriented,” the focus is on specifying the relationship between information stimuli and the decision outcome. Typically, information ”input" is represented in terms of cues to which the decision maker responds. Through the use of statistical procedures (e.g. linear regression), the strength of the stimulus-response relationships can be measured and decision choices can be predicted. While implicit assumptions about cognitive processes are made in such models, the actual nature of the processes remain hidden within an inaccessible ”black box” (Hogarth, 1974). The "process oriented" approach, the other stance adopted by decision making research, attempts to cast light into the ”black box" of outcome oriented studies by examining the thought processes involved in decision making. Accordingly, the intent is to describe hgg decisions are made (Abelson & Levi, 1985). Studies which have attempted to illuminate (or "trace") the processes of decision making have employed several methods. One of the more formal and rigorous methods involves the collection and analysis of decision maker’s verbal protocols. Usually with the aid of an information board (see Payne, 1976), decision makers are asked to ”think aloud" while performing a decision task. The resulting verbal statements are 23 recorded, broken into short task assessment phrases, and then content analyzed for evidence of different decision strategies (Payne & Ragsdale, 1978; Payne, 1976). Through an analysis of the verbal data, the processes by which information input is transformed into decision outcomes can be described. Observationally based studies can also be used to gain insight into the cognitive processes of decision makers. In such studies, observers ask decision makers, during their normal course of work activities, to describe the cognitive processes which were involved in resolving a given decision situation (Mastrofski & Parks, 1990). While this method of collecting process data is often viewed as being less rigorous than verbal protocol analysis, it offers the potential advantage of keeping the study of decision making it its natural environment. It also appears most appropriate when the decision tasks are not naturally well defined or structured. Regardless of the approach however, both ”methods" can provide at least preliminary insight into how decision makers go about making decisions. Decision making studies which have adopted the "outcome" and ”process" approaches are discussed below. The research highlighted in the outcome oriented section focuses specifically on decision making within the criminal justice system. The review of process oriented studies focuses on the psychological literature of process tracing and highlights the contributions a process oriented approach can 24 make to the more traditional outcome oriented inquiry. In each of the sections, the general research propositions and questions which are addressed in this study are developed. Decisions as Outcomes Myriad studies have attempted to identify the case characteristics (stimuli) which influence the decision responses of participants within the justice system. In defining case characteristics, one can distinguish between ”legal" and ”extra-legal" factors. Legal factors include characteristics of the offense such as the amount and type of evidence and the amount of harm done (Nagel, 1983). Extra-legal factors include most often characteristics of the victim and/or offender such as sex, age, race, etc. Since an ideal in the administration of justice is the fair and equal application of the law, it is considered irrational to base decisions on "irrelevant" extra-legal considerations. As explained by Karmen (1984)... The Fourteenth Amendment to the Constitution promises ”equal protection of the law" for all citizens. The standard interpretation of this pledge is that [the criminal justice system] ought to regard factors like social class, race, nationality, religion, and sex as irrelevant to the administration of the law (p. 164). Because of the complexity and uncertainty inherent in many decision making tasks within the criminal justice system however, it is difficult to structure or ”program" decision situations with rules, guidelines, and instructions (Lipsky, 1980; Thompson, 1967). Therefore, since there is often not 25 any method by which extra-legal factors can be absolutely ruled out of decision making, research to understand the stimuli which affect decision making must examine the relative importance of both sets of variables. Victim characteristics and decision making A great deal of previous criminal justice decision making research has demonstrated how characteristics of offenders affect decisions of legal actors (e.g. Platt-Jendrek, 1984; Nagel, 1983; Spohn, Gruhl, & Welch, 1987). However, at the criminal investigation stage, the offender is often unknown to the police and the victim (U.S. Department of Justice, 1988). Therefore, it is more appropriate to examine how the characteristics of xigtims influence decisions. Black’s (1976) theory of the behavior of law provides the foundation for much of the research which has attempted to predict the relationship between victim characteristics and criminal justice decision making. Black presents a series of propositions which attempt to predict the way law behaves, or in specific reference to criminal law, the responses of decision makers within the criminal justice system (e.g., victims, police, judges, etc.).1 Law, as defined by Black, is ”governmental social control" which can vary quantitatively (as well as qualitatively)2 across time, space, and individuals. By the quantity of law, Black refers to the extent to which legal action is initiated, 26 invoked, or applied. For example, an arrest by the police is more law than no arrest, a long prison sentence is more law than a short prison sentence and, in specific reference to this study, an investigation is more law than no investigation. According to Black, the amount of law invoked as a result of a particular incident varies with the perceived seriousness of the incident; with more seriousness corresponding to ”more law." However, unlike other conceptions of seriousness where seriousness is attributable to the objective nature of the act itself (e.g., amount of harm done), seriousness is viewed by Black as a function of the contextual (or social structural) factors of the victim and/or offender. For example, if two homicides occurred and only the characteristics of the victims varied (i.e., offender characteristics were unknown or controlled for), then the perceived seriousness of the incidents (and the amount of law invoked) would vary in the way predicted by the theory. The social structural variables of importance in Black’s theory are: stratification, morphology, culture, organization, and social control. In the discussion which follows, the propositions associated with these variables, as derived by Black, are presented. Previous criminal justice, police (patrol officer), and detective decision making research which has examined these propositions is then reviewed. Stratification. Stratification, as discussed by Black, 27 is "the vertical aspect of social life... the uneven distribution of material conditions of existence” or "inequality of wealth” (p. 11). According to Black, one’s wealth is equatable to one’s position or rank in society. Holding the offender’s rank constant, ”law varies directly with the rank of the victim" (p. 26). Because crimes against the wealthy are viewed as more serious than crimes against the poor, according to Black, "the lower ranks have (or get) less law than the higher ranks” (p. 17). Black adds that ”it is even possible to rank entire neighborhoods. This may be done either according to the distribution of wealth among residents or according to the wealth of the... area itself" (p. 20). Black explicitly states, "the wealthier the victim of a crime, the more likely is an investigation by the police" (p. 27). Accordingly: Victim income influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, wealthy crime victims get more law than poor victims. In addition, according to Black, sex is associated with one’s rank (p. 17) -- with females receiving less law than males. Therefore: Victim sex influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, male crime victims get more law than female crime victims. In examining the relationship between victim income and 28 decision making, Smith and Klein (1984) found that ”the police respond differently to interpersonal disputes depending on the socioeconomic status of the neighborhood in which it occurs” (p. 475). Specifically, the police were more likely to make arrests in low status areas (which were presumably populated with lower status victims). Smith (1987) reported that ”economic status of the neighborhood,” measured by the percent of households with an annual income below $5,000, had a significant impact on police decision making in violent disputes with an arrest more likely to occur in lower status neighborhoods. Black (1970) however, found that the police were less likely to file a felony complaint report when the complainant was a ”blue collar” individual as opposed to a "white collar" individual. In reference to the affect of victim income on detective decision making, Ericson (1981) found that ”cases with high status or otherwise special victims were sometimes given immediate priority because of orders that could ultimately be traceable to the upper echelons of the police organization" (p. 79). Waegel (1981) explained that in burglary cases the victim’s class position had a ”decisive impact" on the amount of attention given to the case. According to Bynum et al. (1982), burglary cases with victims who lived in census tracts with higher median incomes were more likely than victims who lived in tracts with lower incomes to receive extensive investigative attention. This variable was not found to affect decision 29 2k" making in personal offenses however. Concerning the influence of victim gender on decision making, Williams (1978), in a comprehensive examination of the role of the victim in the prosecution of violent offenses, found that cases with female victims were more likely to be rejected by the prosecutor. Similarly, Smith and Klein (1984) and Smith (1987) found that the police were significantly less likely to arrest in situations involving female complainants. .With detective decision making however, Bynum et al. (1982) found that the victim’s gender did not have an effect on decision making in either property or personal offenses. Mgrphglggx. Morphology is defined as ”the horizontal aspect of social life, the distribution of people in relation to one another, including their division of labor, networks of interaction, intimacy, and integration” (p. 37). Individuals "may participate more or less in social life itself. Some participate fully and usefully; others stay at the margin, hardly involved at all" (p. 48). ”Some people work; others idle or loiter” (p. 48). According to Black, employment status serves as an indicator of integration. Hold constant the offender, ”and law varies directly with the integration of the victim" (p. 53). Black claims that crimes against integrated (employed) victims are viewed as more serious than crimes against non-integrated (not employed) victims, and as a result, those who are non-integrated get less law than those who are integrated. 30 "The closer to the center he is... the more extensive is an investigation of his problem" (p. 53). Accordingly: Victim employment status influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, victims who are employed get more law than victims who are not employed. In addition, ”people vary in the degree to which they participate in one another’s lives” (p. 40). ”It is possible to measure relational distance in many ways, including the scope, frequency, and length of interaction between people, the age of their relationship, and the nature and number of links between them in a social network" (p. 41). According to Black, ”law is inactive among intimates, increasing as the distance between people increases" (p. 41). Therefore: The victim-suspect relationship influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, victims who are not acquainted with the suspect get more law than victims who are acquainted with the suspect. Previous research, to one degree or another, has addressed both of these predictions. Only one study in the} literature, Bynum et al. (1982), examined the impact of victim employment status on decision making. They found that employment status did not affect the amount of effort devoted to follow-up investigations. The victim-suspect relationship has been found to influence decision making. According to Schmidt and Steury (1989), domestic assault cases in which victims shared a 31 dwelling with the offender and were sexually intimate with the offender prior to the assault were less likely to be continued. Similarly, Albonetti (1986) discovered that victims who were acquainted with the suspect were more likely to have their cases discontinued than were victims who were strangers to the suspect. According to Black (1971), the police were more likely to arrest an offender when he was a stranger to the victim as opposed to when the offender and the victim were friends, acquaintances, or relatives (also see Smith & Visher, 1981; LaFave, 1965; Friedrich, 1977). Similarly, Worden and Pollitz (1984) found that in domestic disturbance situations, the offender was more likely to be arrested if the disputing parties were not married. No studies have examined the impact of the victim-offender relationship on detective decision making. Cultu e. Black defines culture as "the symbolic aspect of social life including expressions of what is true, good, and beautiful” (p. 61). Because of variance in ideas, information, languages, and customs, some societies and "individuals have more culture than do others" (p. 64). While education is presented as the primary indicator of culture, Black also suggests that certain groups in a society are closer to the mainstream of culture or are more "conventional." Holding the offender’s characteristics constant, "law varies directly with the conventionality of the victim" (p. 70). For example, a crime against a 32 cultural minority is claimed to be viewed as less serious than a crime committed against a cultural majority and consequently, it is predicted that cultural minorities receive less law than cultural majorities. Therefore: Victim race influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, white crime victims get more law than non-white victims. Concerning the influence of racial identification, Smith (1987) found that the police were significantly less likely to make an arrest when the situation involved a black complainant. However, Smith and Klein (1984) did not find such a relationship. As for detective decision making, Waegel’s (1981) observations led him to believe that the victim’s race had an impact on the amount of attention given to the case. However, Bynum et al. (1982) found that victim race did not affect the extent of effort spent in a follow-up investigation. Organization. Black refers to organization as ”the corporate aspect of social life, the capacity for collective action” (p. 85). "Measures of organization include the presence and number of administrative officers, the centralization and continuity of decision making, and the quantity of collective action itself" (p. 85). "Any group is, by definition, more organized than an individual on his own" (p. 86). According to Black, "law varies directly with organization” (p. 86) and ”the more organized the victim of a crime, the more serious is the offense” (p. 95). 33 Therefore: The type of victim influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, businesses get more law than non- businesses. Only one study in the literature, Albonetti (1986), assessed the impact that the type of victim (individual or organized collective) had on decision making. In this study, the type of victim did not affect the decision of whether or not to continue prosecution at the post-indictment stage. Social Cont; 1. Black describes social control as ”the normative aspect of social life. It defines and responds to deviant behavior specifying what ought to be... It divides people into those who are respectable and those who are not” (p. 105). Respectability refers to one’s normative status, the degree to which a person has been subject to law and other forms of social control. According to Black, "law varies directly with respectability” (p. 112) with more respectable victims receiving more law than non-respectable victims. Since an indicator of victim respectability was not available in the data collected here, a hypothesis relating to this component of Black’s theory could not be formulated or tested. In addition to the victim characteristics identified by Black, previous research has also suggested that the Preferences, or wishes, of the victim are important in 34 predicting decision responses of criminal justice actors. In reference to police decision making, when the victim prefers no arrest, the police will likely comply with the request. In fact, in a study by Smith and Klein (1984), the strongest determinant of an arrest was the complainant’s request to have the offender arrested (also see Berk & Loseke, 1981; Black, 1980; Friedrich, 1977; Lundman, Sykes, & Clark, 1978; LaFave, 1965). Hence, on the basis of previous research, one could expect that: Victim desire for formal action influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, victims who desire formal action get more law than victims who do not desire formal action. Finally, it is of interest in this study to explore the impact of victim age on decision making. Only Bynum et al. (1982) included age as a predictor of decision outcomes. They found that victim age did not influence the amount of effort devoted to follow-up investigations. Due to the lack of "theory" and previous research on this issue, a hypothesis on this issue is not specified. In sum, while the relationship between victim income, victim-suspect relationship, and victim preferences and criminal justice decision making is generally well established and congruent with Black’s theory, the impact of gender, racial identification, employment status, and victim type is not. Furthermore, when focusing exclusively on the research which has examined the impact of victim 35 characteristics on detective decision making in follow-up investigations, it becomes apparent that the empirical evidence is scant and often contradictory. As a result, theoretical benefits could be realized from the provision of additional evidence on these issues. Offense characteristics and decision making Along with victim characteristics, a host of studies have examined the impact of offense (”legal”) characteristics on criminal justice decision making. These studies, either implicitly or explicitly, have tested an alternative conception of seriousness from that proposed by Black (1976); specifically, that seriousness of the offense is reflected not through the social structural characteristics of the participants but through the nature of the offense -- most commonly, the extent of injury, the amount of property loss, and the involvement of a weapon. Also considered an offense characteristic, but not reflective of ”seriousness,” is the strength of the evidence. In the discussion which follows, research findings concerning the influence of offense characteristics on decision making are reviewed and, on the basis of this :review, hypotheses are developed. De r I ur . Several studies have addressed the eexpectation that more injury is reflective of a more serious C>ffense, and therefore deserving of "more law.” According t1: Schmidt and Steury (1989), victims who suffered moderate 36 orsevef IcharS police 1980; E 1987) 1 if one lKeel Bynum have t inveS' inclu I relat the 1 inJI VieI tha Cut avg lo: the in or severe injury were more likely to see the case result in a charge against the defendant. However, in relation to police decision making, numerous studies (Berk & Loseke, 1980; Smith & Klein, 1984; Worden & Pollitz, 1984; Smith, 1987) found that the likelihood of arrest did not increase if one of the disputing parties was injured (but see Waaland & Keeley, 1985). In regard to detective decision making, Bynum et al. (1982) found that the degree of injury did not have a significant impact on the extent of the follow-up investigation in personal offenses ("injury" was not included in the analysis of property offenses). In accord with the original expectation concerning the relationship between degree of injury and decision making, the following is hypothesized: Degree of injury influences the amount of law invoked as a result of the criminal incident. Specifically, cases which involve more injury will get more law than cases which involve less injury. WW- Similar to the degree of injury, one might expect cases with much property loss to be viewed as more serious, and more deserving of attention, than cases which involve little property loss. Adams and Cutshall (1987) and Bynum et al. (1982) are the only available studies which have examined the effect of property loss on decision making. Adams and Cutshall (1987) found the value of property loss to be of "marginal significance" in the decision to prosecute. Bynum et al. (1982) found 37 that the extent of property loss did not have a significant impact on the extent to which property offenses were investigated ("loss" was not included in the analysis of personal offenses). On the basis of the original expectation, it is hypothesized that: The value of property loss influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, cases which involve much property loss get more law than cases which involve little property loss. We on e. Several studies have examined the impact of weapon use on decision making. The rationale for this examination is that crimes which involve a weapon have a potential for greater personal harm and are therefore "more serious" and deserving of increased attention. In a study conducted by Schmidt and Steury (1987), it was found that cases which involved the use of a weapon in the commission of the crime were more likely to proceed to court. However, Nagel (1983) found that the commission of a crime with a weapon did not affect the pre-trial release decision. In reference to police decision making, Smith and Klein (1984) and Smith (1987) found that the presence of a weapon at a dispute did not significantly increase the probability of arrest. No studies of detective decision making have examined this relationship. In accord with the underlying rationale for this examination, the following is suggested: Weapon use influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, crimes committed with a weapon get more law than crimes not committed with a weapon. 38 Egiggggg. Adams and Cutshall (1987), Albonetti (1986), Burnstein, Kelly, and Doyle (1977), Schmidt and Steury (1989), and Forst and Brosi (1977) found that the strength of the evidence was an important factor in the prosecutor’s determination of whether or not to issue charges or continue prosecution of an offender; the stronger the evidence, the more likely charges would be pursued. A similar relationship between strength of the evidence and disposition has been found concerning the decisions to release on recognizance or bail (Frazier, Bock, & Henretta, 1980), sentence (Platt-Jendrek, 1984), and release on parole (Heinz, Heinz, Senderowitz, & Vance, 1976). Previous research also indicates that evidence plays a major role in the police decision to arrest -- "the stronger the evidence in the field situation, the more likely is an arrest" (Black, 1971). Specifically, Black (1971) found that police were more likely to make an arrest when they actually witnessed a criminal incident as opposed to merely learning of the incident through a citizen report. Prior research on the criminal investigation process suggests that detective decision making is also affected by the strength of evidence. For example, in the seminal research by Greenwood et al. (1977), it is stated that "investigators choose the [cases] they will work by considering... whether sufficient leads are present to indicate that the chances of clearing the crime are high” (p. 110). The observational studies conducted by Sanders 39 (1977) and Ericson (1981) also come to the same general conclusion. Eck (1983) provides additional support to this conclusion by finding that robberies receive more investigative attention than burglaries because first, robberies are viewed as more serious than burglaries and second, robberies have a greater potential for better evidence. Additionally, Bynum et al. (1982) found that the amount of evidence was strongly related to the extent of investigative effort in property offenses (i.e., more evidence led to a more extensive investigation) but not in personal offenses. In the Bynum et al. (1982) study, "amount of evidence" was measured as an interval level index through the presence of ten solvability factors: was there a witness? can suspect(s) be named? can suspect(s) be located? can suspect(s) be described? can suspect(s) be identified? is the stolen property traceable? is there a significant M.O. present? is there physical evidence present? has evidence technician work been performed? In accord with the previous research which has found a relationship between strength of evidence and decision making responses, it expected that: Evidence influences the amount of law invoked as a result of a criminal incident. Specifically, the stronger the evidence, the more likely the case will receive more law (where ”more law" equals ”selected for an investigationf). In regard to the amount of time spent on the follow-up investigation, one might expect that when there is weak 40 evidence in the case there will be little time spent on the investigation because the detective does not expect much chance of solution regardless of the activities performed (Eck, 1979). Similarly, when the evidence associated with a crime is very strong, the detective may not need to spend much time on the investigation because the suspect can easily be identified and arrested. However, cases in which the evidence is of moderate strength may lead to much time being spent on the investigation because the investigation has a reasonable chance of solution if further information becomes available. Therefore: Crimes with evidence of moderate strength get more law than than crimes with weak or strong evidence (where "more law” equals "more time spent on an investigation"). In sum, similar to the research on the relationship between victim characteristics and decision making, there is empirical support for the claim that criminal justice decision making is influenced by offense characteristics. While one might infer the same to be true in regard to detective decision making, the relationship here is generally not well established. As a consequence, there is a need for additional research to assess the impact of offense characteristics on detective decision making. Decisions as Processes The process tracing approach to decision making allows for the examination of the actual cognitive processes 41 invoked to perform a decision task. Inferences are made as to the nature of these processes not on the basis of mathematical computations as with statistical models, but rather on subjects’ search patterns and/or verbal reports of the cognitive steps taken in order to make a decision. Currently, in the criminal justice decision making literature, there are no studies which have attempted to cultivate such data. This is unfortunate because process data would contribute additional insight into the complexities of detective decision making and ultimately further our understanding of the investigative process. Process tracing data have been collected from decision makers performing various decision tasks including consumer product selections (Payne & Ragsdale, 1978; Olshavsky, 1979), clinical diagnosis (Einhorn, Kleinmuntz, & Kleinmuntz, 1979; Hogarth, 1974), securities selection (Clarkson, 1962), and problem solving type tasks such as verbal analogies (Grundin, 1980), geometry theorems (Greeno, 1976) and playing chess (DeGroot, 1975). Many of these studies used an information board to present the stimuli for the decision task. An information board consists of a matrix of alternatives (the thing to choose; e.g., an apartment) and dimensions (information about the thing to choose; e.g., cost of rent) for a particular decision situation. Information boards are either mechanically operated (Payne, 1976) or computerized (Gilliland, 1990). With mechanically operated information 42 boards, cards with pieces of information are placed face down to form the matrix of information and then subjects are asked to manually turn over the cards on which information is desired. Computerized information boards provide for the display and search of information by pressing computer terminal command keys. Regardless of the type of information board used, information search patterns can be observed through manifested actions and supplemented with verbal reports of thoughts and actions. The majority of process oriented studies have been conducted in laboratory settings with student subjects.3 As a result, this methodology has not been well tested in field settings. Given this factor along with the observation that detectives (and the police in general) are protective of their work, suspicious of outsiders, and generally non-cooperative (cf. Ericson, 1981), an issue of concern in this study is whether it is feasible to collect process data through the use of an information board from detectives in the field setting (this issue is discussed in 4 the final chapter). The process tracing literature has identified two decision making strategies -- linear (compensatory) and non-linear (non-compensatory) (Payne, 1976). An individual who uses a linear strategy in making a decision examines a constant number of dimensions across alternatives, mentally assigns weights to each of the dimensions, sums the negative 43 and positive weights for each alternative, then chooses the alternative with the highest overall score (Payne, 1976). A linear strategy of decision making is evident in the following verbal protocol obtained from a student selecting a hypothetical apartment: O.K., we have an A and a B. First look at the rent for both of them. The rent for A is $170 and the rent for B is $140. $170 is a little steep, but it might have a low noise level. So we’ll check A’s noise level. A’s noise level is low. We’ll go back to B’s noise level. It’s high. Gee, I can’t really very well study with a lot of noise. So I’ll ask myself the question, is it worth spending that extra $30 a month for, to be able to study in my apartment (Payne, 1976, p. 378). Apparently for this individual less expensive rent could compensate for a higher noise level in selecting an apartment. Conversely, with a non-linear strategy, a variable number of dimensions across alternatives are examined (Payne, 1976) and "a low score on one dimension cannot be compensated for by a high score on another dimension" (Ford et al., 1989, p. 77). Within the non-linear strategy, several ”substrategies" of decision making have been identified -- conjunctive, disjunctive, lexographic, and elimination by aspect (Svenson, 1979; Olshavsky, 1979; Payne, 1976). A ggnjgngtigg strategy is used when the decision maker assigns an acceptable value to each important dimension, and if the acceptable value is not obtained for each dimension, then the alternative is eliminated. With the digjnggtixg strategy, the decision maker once again establishes acceptable values for each important dimension. 44 However, the alternative which is chosen exceeds the minimum value on at least one dimension while all of the other alternatives would be equal or fall below the minimum value. With the lexicographic strategy, dimensions are rank-ordered in terms of importance. An alternative is then selected based on the ranking of the most important dimension. Finally, a decision maker who uses the gliminatign_bz_§§pggt strategy once again rank-orders dimensions within each alternative but the alternatives in which a dimension does not meet a minimum value are eliminated from consideration. A non-linear (elimination by aspect) decision strategy is apparent in the protocol below: Since we have a whole bunch here, I’m going to go across the top and see which noise levels are high. If there are any high ones, I’ll reject them immediately (Payne, 1976, p. 375). Apparently, an attractive dimension of an alternative, such as inexpensive rent, could not compensate for a high noise level. Statistical models of decision making assume that decision makers use linear decision strategies. Research has shown, however, that under certain conditions this assumption is inaccurate. For example, increased task complexity, generally defined in terms of the amount of information available to the decision maker, has been associated with the use of non-linear, non-compensatory decision strategies (Payne, 1976; Onken, Hastie, & Revelle, 1985; Olshavsky, 1979). Non-linear strategies serve to 45 limit the amount of information to be processed by the decision maker thus simplifying the decision task (Onken et al., 1985). These simplifying strategies may be used early in the task and then, when some of the alternatives have been eliminated from consideration, the decision maker may switch to linear strategies (Payne, 1976). Given the amount of information available to detectives when making decisions, and therefore the seemingly complex nature of the decision tasks, one might expect that detectives employ, to a large extent, non-linear decision making strategies. Previous research on investigative decision making has not addressed this expectation. Therefore, another question addressed in this study is the extent to which detectives use linear (vs. non-linear) strategies in making decisions. Process tracing research has also illustrated that decision making involves search processes -- processes which vary in depth, sequence, content, and latency (Ford et al., 1989). Of direct concern in this study are depth of search and content of search. D;pth_g£_§gangh refers to the proportion of total information examined prior to rendering a decision. Through an examination of a decision maker’s depth of search, it is possible to infer the existence of linear or non-linear decision making strategies (Payne, 1976). For example, searching a large proportion of information implies a linear strategy while the search of a small proportion indicates the use of a non-linear 46 ,, ILIA . . .W.‘flb.‘.j Ill. . strategy. In addition, as explained by Payne (1976), searching a constant number of dimensions (information) across alternatives implies the use of a linear strategy while searching a variable number of dimensions across alternatives suggests that the decision maker was using non-linear strategies (also see Ford et al., 1989). Therefore, it is useful to examine the proportion of case information searched by detectives in making decisions. Qontont of soonon refers to the specific elements of information examined by a subject in making a decision. From an examination of search content, one can specify the dimensions upon which decisions are (at least partially) based. For example, Payne and Ragsdale (1978) attempted to describe the extent to which certain consumer product attributes (e.g., price) were mentioned (and presumably considered) in making decisions to purchase grocery items. Similarly, through an analysis of detectives’ search patterns and verbal reports, insight into the case factors most often considered in decision making can be obtained and the influence of other factors on decision making can be illuminated. An examination of search content offers an alternative means by which the hypotheses concerning case characteristics and decision making can be examined. Accordingly, the process data will be used to identify the information elements (case characteristics) which are most important to detectives in making decisions. 47 Summary Chapter Two has provided the theoretical and analytic foundation for this study. The case selection decision and the time allocation decision were presented as the two major decisions of detectives. The outcome and process oriented approaches were identified as the two approaches used in the study of decision making. Propositions which predict the decision responses of detectives were derived from Black’s theory of the behavior of law and previous research, and research questions concerning the cognitive processes of detectives were developed on the basis of the process tracing literature. 48 Footnotes As Hembroff (1987) illustrates, the theory is not limited to predicting the actions of individuals within the criminal justice system. Rather, the theory predicts when and how much law will be invoked by any individual in all types of situations. While the decisions may differ by the actor, all can be equated with "more or less law" as discussed by Black. Qualitatively, law can take several forms: penal, compensatory, therapeutic, or conciliatory. Clarkson, 1962; Hogarth, 1974; and Payne and Ragsdale, 1978, are notable exceptions. As discussed earlier in this chapter and in detail in Chapter Four, along with the information board as a method of collecting process data, the less rigorous method of field observations and interviews was also used to collect data on how detectives make decisions. The use of this method in this manner is also a move into unchartered territory (Mastrofski & Parks, 1990). 49 CHAPTER THREE RESEARCH SITE Chapter Three contains a description of the study site. The city in which the police department is located is briefly described, the features of the police department are discussed, and the mechanics of the criminal investigation process within the department are outlined. The City The City of Landau (a pseudonym) is a medium sized midwestern city with a (1990) population of approximately 130,000 people, approximately 33 percent of which are non-white. The greater metropolitan area has a population of approximately 450,000 people. The city is located on 34 square miles of land. In 1980, the city contained 49,516 households. Landau is administered by a council-mayor form of government. The major employers in the City of Landau are manufacturing and assembly plants, state government, retail, and health care. The unemployment rate in 1986 was 7.0%. 50 The LandauPolice Department At the time of this study, the Landau Police Department employed 245 sworn officers, eleven non-sworn officers, and 91 civilians. In 1989, the department responded to 128,442 calls for service. The operating budget for 1989 was $12,388,532. In Figure 1, the organizational chart of the L.P.D. is illustrated. As seen, the department is managed by a chief of police. A deputy chief and assistant chief are responsible for the two major operating components of the department -- staff services and field services, respectively. The staff services component consists of the administrative support division (administrative services, personnel and training) and the operations support division (records, radio maintenance, and communications center). The field services component consists of the uniform division (special services and patrol) and the investigations division. Each division within the department is supervised by a captain. During this study, 30 of the 245 sworn officers in the department were assigned to the investigations division. Twenty-two of these personnel were the rank of detective, one was a patrol officer temporarily assigned to the division, four were sergeants, two were lieutenants, and there was one captain. All of the detectives worked primarily a fixed shift of 8 a.m. to 4 p.m., Monday through 51 H UHVKK~ChS~ :2: $05me E23228 3:8 use 3858» 525:2. SE: as: :2: :2: I 5:88: 8E2 2:5: 88%: 3% as: 5229. war a. l2: '2: 55:. lo leg _ Ema .— fl—J mzozézgzos .52: 93 3.913318% a _ 299% E0; :2: E 295m 92.23: m .mzzOmmwa m>=_o mzo=z_ 206.55 E82: mzozéwmo mmOSmww mum/pm mmOSmwm OIEE 52 §§ .2255. bio Erma 8:2 mo bio :2th 8:2 mo bio 5;: WE 8 ono se<=o qcH you couomHom omeo u «H mHsmcoHuoHom umccommoraHuoH> u h huummoum :oHovm uo oaHo> w u mH oEoosH aHuoH> u w oHnoHMHucocH manomoum u NH om< EHDUH> n m csocz momma mHoHsm> u as msumum ucmesoaaam sHuoH> u a nonHuomoo oHoH£o> u oH doom EHuoH> u m oococH>m Heonwzm n m xom EHHUH> u N :oHuofiuousH poommsm u m om>9 EHHOH> n H cousmfioo on uoccco usmHoHuuooo “sumo msHumHE * oo.H Ho. No.l NH. SH. eo. mm. Hm. bo.l mo.l No. mo. HH. mo.l eH oo.H mo. 00. Ho. mH. HH.I 0H.I mN. HH. mo. Ho.I wo.l mo. NH oo.H no. No. no.l mo.l OH.I mo.l Ho. ho. Ho.I Ho.I mo.l NH oo.H we. Ho.l NH. no. mo. «0. Ho. mo.l mo. mo.l HH oo.H oo. NH. 00. mo. mo. mo. no. mo. 50.! OH oo.H wo.I no.I mo. «H. NH. 0H.I No.l #0. m oo.H Nm. 0H.I NH.I no.l mH. NH. HH.I w oo.H 0H.I hH.I oo.I eH. mH. MH.I h oo.H SH. mo. 0H.I Ho. No. w oo.H oo.I HH.I ho.I # m oo.H mo.I mo.I no. e oo.H mo. * m oo.H ¢ N oo.H H eH NH NH HH 0H m w h w m e m N H Ahmmflzv mmHm4HUMDm HH< mom mmqm UZOS¢ mfizmHUHmmmOU ZOHB¢HmMMOU N mflm<fi 95 for a follow-up while 183 out of 244 cases (75%) with a victim-offender relationship were selected for a follow-up investigation. Therefore, with a relationship between the victim and offender, the case was more likely to get assigned. It is of some consequence to note however, that the relationship between the suspect information variable and the victim-offender relationship variable is .32; when there was a relationship between the victim and offender, a name of the suspect was usually provided. None of the other correlation coefficients between the independent variables and the dependent variable are of appreciable strength. Because it was of primary interest to isolate the relative influence of the victim and offense variables on the case selection decision, multivariate statistical procedures were used. In Table 3, the coefficients, standard errors, t-values, and standardized weights from probit and OLS regression analyses are presented. Although it is generally considered inappropriate to conduct OLS regression analyses on a dichotomous dependent variable, two anomalies in the probit results make consideration of OLS regression analyses necessary. First, the derivative at the mean2 for strength of suspect information is quite small (.05) yet significant at the p < .01 level. Second, the derivative at the mean for suspect vehicle plate known is extremely large (.72) but not significant (p = .989). It appears that these seemingly 96 TABLE 3 PROBIT (AND MULTIPLE REGRESSION) ANALYSIS OF BURGLARY CASE SELECTION AS A FUNCTION OF VICTIM AND OFFENSE CHARACTERISTICS (regression results in parentheses) Independent Standard derivative Variables Coefficient Error t (beta) ( .012) (.028) ( .406) ( .01) Victim Sex .054 .125 .429 .02 (-.013) (.029) ( -.466) (-.01) Victim Race .136 .142 .955 .05 (-.036) (.032) (-1.125) (-.03) Victim Emp Status .293 .170 1.721 .11* ( .053) (.038) ( 1.398) ( .04) Victim Age -.004 .004 -.870 .00 ( .000) (.001) ( .345) ( .01) Victim Income .000 .000 -.497 .00 Victim-Off. Rel .489 .069 7.089 .19** ( .423) (.029) (14.396) ( .39**) Suspect Information 1.750 .152 11.495 .05** ( .354) (.022) (16.020) ( .44**) Physical Evidence .342 .130 2.625 .13** ( .098) (.032) ( 3.075) ( .08**) Vehicle Described .931 .310 3.000 .36** ( .298) (.073) ( 4.081) ( .12**) Vehicle Plate Known 5.170 386.788 .013 .72 (-0015) (0154) ( -0098) ( .00) Property Id’able -.040 .089, -.452 -.01 ( .045) (.034) ( 1.304) ( .03) $ Value of Property .000 .000 -1.459 .00 ( .000) (.000) ( 3.095) ( .08*#) 2 Pseudo R a .32 2 (Adjusted R 8 .46) Significance = .00 (Significance = .00) 1 N = 857 1 (N 8 857) * p<.05; ** p<.01 (one-tailed test) 1 the mean of each variable was substituted for missing data 97 unreliable coefficients are at least a partial result of multicollinearity.3 As a result of these anomalous probit results, the attractiveness of the multiple regression analyses increases. In short, it appears that given the nature of these data, the regression results (beta weights) are less biased than the probit results (derivative at the means). It is of comfort to note that the probit and regression analyses are quite similar in terms of those factors which display a significant impact on the case selection decision.4 As seen in Table 3, both sets of results indicate that strength of suspect information, the presence of a victim-offender relationship, description of a suspect’s vehicle, and availability of physical evidence exert a significant impact on the case selection decision. The only inconsistencies in the results are that the dollar value of the property loss is significant in the regression analysis but not in the probit analysis while employment status of the victim is significant in the probit analysis but not in the regression. According to the regression results, the largest impact on the case selection decision is exerted by the strength of suspect information; the stronger the suspect information, the more likely the case was selected for an investigation (b = .44; p < .01). Presence of a relationship between the victim and offender also exerts a significant influence; cases where a relationship existed were more likely to be ‘98 selected (b = .39; p < .01). The variable with the third strongest impact was a description of the suspect’s vehicle; when this information was known, the case was more likely to be selected (b = .12; p < .01). The final two variables which display a statistically significant effect on case selection are presence of physical evidence and the dollar value of the stolen property. When physical evidence was available or when more value loss was involved, the case was more likely to get selected (b = .08; p (.01 for each). On the basis of these analyses, the following hypotheses are supported: Hypothesis 7: Cases with a higher value of stolen property are more likely than cases with lesser value to be selected for an investigation. Hypothesis 9a: Cases with stronger suspect information are more likely than cases with weaker suspect information to be selected for an investigation. Hypothesis 9c: Cases with physical evidence are more likely than cases without physical evidence to be selected for an investigation. Hypothesis 9d: Cases with a suspect vehicle description are more likely than cases without a vehicle description to be selected for an investigation. In addition, the opposite of the following hypothesis is supported: Hypothesis 4: Victims who do not have a relationship with the suspect are more likely than victims who do have a relationship with the suspect to have their cases selected for an investigation. 99 Time Allocation in Burglary Investigations Table 4 presents the frequency distribution for the independent variables (victim and offense) and dependent variable (time spent on the follow-up investigation) on the subset of burglary cases which were selected for a follow-up investigation (N = 317). Table 4 also reflects the coding scheme used in the bivariate and multivariate analyses. As illustrated in Table 4, 252 of the 317 burglary cases (80%) which received a follow-up investigation involved victims who were individuals, while 65 of the 317 cases (20%) involved businesses. A slight majority of the selected cases, 138 of 252 (55%), involved female crime victims. Most of the cases, 169 of 252 (67%), involved victims who were white, and employed 126 of 172 (73%). The mean age for the burglary victims who had their cases selected was 36 years and the mean "income" was $15,687. In 45 of 308 cases (15%), the victim did not desire investigative effort. A relationship between the victim and the suspected offender was present in 184 of 310 cases (59%) selected for an investigation. It is also evident from Table 4 that while 96 of the 317 selected cases (30%) contained moderate suspect information, 221 of 317 (70%) cases contained weak or strong suspect information. Specifically, 155 of the 317 cases (49%) contained weak suspect information while 66 of 317 (21%) contained strong information. Physical evidence was 100 TABLE 4 INDEPENDENT AND DEPENDENT VARIABLES: VALUES AND DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS BY "SELECTED" BURGLARIES # Variable Value N % Victim Type 0=Individual 252 80 1=Business 65 20 Victim Sex 0=Male 114 45 1=Female 138 55 Victim Race 0=White 169 67 l=Non-white 83 33 Victim Employment 0=Not employed 46 27 Status 1=Employed 126 73 Victim Age N: 249 (in years) X= 36 SD= 13.8 Min/Max: 17-95 Victim Income N: 247 (S/year) X: 15,687 SD= 4,785 Min/Max= 7,260-31,672 Victim Desires 0=No 45 15 Effort 1=Yes 263 85 Victim- Offender Relationship 0=No 126 41 Present 1=Yes 184 59 Strength of 0=Weak/Strong 221 70 Suspect Info 1=Moderate 96 30 Physical 0=No 250 79 Evidence 1=Yes 67 21 Suspect Vehicle 0=No 292 92 Described 1=Yes 25 8 Suspect Vehicle 0=No 310 98 Lic Plate Known 1=Yes 7 2 Stolen Property 0=No 270 85 Identifiable 1=Yes 46 15 Weapon Used 0=No Q Q 1=Yes 0 9 Degree of 0=No injury 0 Q Injury 1=Minor Injury 9 Q 2=Serious Injury 0 9 Value of Stolen N: 317 Property 3:. 1,243 (in dollars) SD= 2,680 Min/Max= 0-30,000 Time spent N3 317 on Follow-up x: 3.7 Investigation SD= 4.6 (in hours) Min/Max= .1-52.5 # missing data are excluded from table 8 variable not appropriate for burglaries 101 I'ILI- FINN». .hllb‘rn I E available in 67 of the 317 cases (21%). In 25 of the 317 selected cases (8%), a suspect’s vehicle was described and in 7 of 317 cases (2%) a license plate number was known. In 270 of 316 cases (85%) the stolen property was not identifiable. The mean value of the stolen property was $1,243 with a range of zero to $30,000. Finally, the mean amount of time spent on burglary follow-up investigations was 3.7 hours with a range of .1 hours to 52.5 hours. To gain an understanding of how time was spent on investigations, the activities performed in each investigation were recorded from the follow-up reports. Table 5 contains these findings. Briefly, it is seen that the most common activity performed in burglary follow-up investigations was interviewing victims. In 213 of the 317 investigations (67%), the victim was interviewed at least once. The second most frequently performed activity was interviewing others who were not directly involved in the crime in question (e.g., mother of suspect, pawn shop personnel, parole officer, etc.); in 126 of 317 cases (40%), this activity was performed. The third most common activity was interviewing suspects; in 116 of the 317 cases (37%) a suspect was interviewed at least once. The remainder of the activities, rank-ordered in frequency, are: searched computer files (23%), consulted prosecutor (21%), interviewed witnesses (20%), submitted physical evidence (15%), searched crime scene (6%), conducted photo line-up 102 TABLE 5 FREQUENCY OF DETECTIVE ACTIVITIES IN BURGLARY FOLLOW-UP INVESTIGATIONS (N=317) Investigations in Which Activity Activity was Performed N i Victim Interviewed 213 67 Others Interviewed 126 40 Suspect Interviewed 116 37 Computer Files Searched 72 23 Prosecutor Consulted 67 21 Witness(es) Interviewed 62 20 Physical Evidence Submitted 47 15 Crime Scene Searched 18 6 Photo Line-up Conducted l6 5 Witness Canvass Conducted 15 5 Informants Interviewed 7 2 Physical Line-up Conducted 6 2 Mug-shot Books Shown 4 1 103 (5%), canvassed for witnesses (5%), interviewed informants (2%), conducted physical line-up (2%), and showed mug shot books (1%). Table 6 presents the correlation coefficients (r) among the independent variables and the dependent variable for those burglaries selected for a follow-up investigation. It is seen that several of the coefficients between the independent variables are of at least moderate strength (.51, .34, .32), however, none appear to approach "dangerous” proportions in terms of collinearity. As for the variables related to the time spent in the follow-up investigation, the dollar value of the stolen property is the strongest (.29). The higher the value of the stolen property, the more likely more time was spent on the investigation. Although not displayed, a cross- tabulation procedure showed that 36 of 163 cases (22%) which involved under $300 of stolen property received over 3.7 hours (the mean) of investigative time. Twenty-seven of 82 cases (33%) which involved $300 to $1,300 of stolen property received over 3.7 hours. Twenty-nine of 72 cases (40%) which had over $1,300 of property taken received over 3.7 hours of investigative time. Three variables, suspect information, physical evidence, and victim income, are all positively related to the amount of time spent (r = .21). With suspect information, a crosstab procedure demonstrated that 34 of 155 cases (22%) 104 mHzECOHuoHom woOGEMHOIEHu0H> n N coHuemHumo>cH so ucomm oaHa n mH uuouum mouHmom EHu0H> n h muuomoum GSHoum mo osHe> w u «H oEoocH aHuoH> u o mHanuHuemeH Nunoaoum n HH one sHuoH> u m :zocz munHm SHOHEN> u «H msumum unmamoHasm sHuoH> u e conHuomoO mHoH£o> u HH doom BHuoH> u n mocmcH>N Hconmsm u OH xom EHuoH> n N :oHuesuoucH poommsm n m mama aHuoH> u H cousmEoo on pocsoo pcoHoHuuooo «Mano mchmHa * oo.H NN. HO.I ON. OH. HN. HN. NH.I NH. HN. NH. OO. OH.I NN.I ho. mH oo.H HO.I OO. 00. Nm. eO.I hH.I HH. em. vN. NO. NO.I mO.I mo. eH oo.H No. Ho. eo.l No. eH.I ho. vo.l No. NH. HO.I «0.! NO.I NH OO.H Hm. NO.I HO.I mO.I OO. eH. mO. HO. hO.I NO. NO.I NH OO.H HO.I HO. OH.I NO.I NH. NH. mo. eO. NO. NO.I HH oo.H NO. NH.I OH. NH. HN. OH. «H.I mO.I mo. OH OO.H Om.I NH. HO. NO. NO. OO.I bH.I HH. m OO.H NN.I OH.I HN.I OH.I ON. «H. NH.I N oo.H NH. OH. NH. NH.I NH.I NH. O oo.H NH. HH. NH.I NO. no. N OO.H NO.I mO.I NH.I * m OO.H bO.I HN.I NO. e OO.H NO. * m OO.H * N oo.H H mH OH NH NH HH OH O N h o m e m N H AhHmflzv mmHm¢HUMDm sameumflmm: mom mWHm ozczd mfizmHUHmmmOU ZOHB¢HfiMMOU o Manda 105 with weak suspect information received more than 3.7 hours of time. Similarly, in cases which contained strong suspect information, 14 of 66 (21%) received over 3.7 hours of time. However, in cases which contained moderate suspect information, 44 of 96 (46%) received more than 3.7 hours of investigative time. In addition, when suspect information is treated as three distinct categories and then correlated with the amount of time spent on the investigation (in raw form) the correlation coefficient drops to .03 from .21. With this evidence, there is at least initial support for the theoretical expectation that cases with moderate suspect information receive more time than those cases with weak or strong suspect information. More importantly, there is justification for combining weak and strong suspect information into one analytic category for the multivariate analyses. To assess the relative impact of the independent variables on the dependent variable, an OLS multiple regression procedure was used. Table 7 contains the results of these analyses. Although the primary intent of these analyses was to test a limited set of hypotheses which predict the amount of time spent in an investigation, it is of interest to note that 17 percent of the variation in time spent on burglary investigations is accounted for by victim and offense characteristics. As seen in Table 7, the greatest impact on time spent is 106 TABLE 7 MULTIPLE REGRESSION OF TIME SPENT ON BURGLARY FOLLOW-UP INVESTIGATIONS AS A FUNCTION OF VICTIM AND OFFENSE CHARACTERISTICS Independent Standardized Variables Beta Victim Type .03 Victim Sex -.04 Victim Race -.07 Victim Employment Status —.06 Victim Age .06 Victim Income .05 Victim Desires Effort .08 Victim-Offender Relationship .00 Suspect Information .19** Physical Evidence .09* Vehicle Described .00 Vehicle Plate Known .19** Property Identifiable -.03 $ Value of Stolen Property .22*4 Multiple R .46 2 Adjusted R .17 F 5.73 Significance .00 1 N 317 * p<.05; ** p<.01 (one-tailed test) 1 the mean of each variable was substituted for missing data 107 exerted by the dollar value of the stolen property; cases with greater loss were more likely to have more time devoted to them (b= .22; p < .01). Strength of suspect information also has a significant impact -- cases with moderate information were more likely to receive more time than cases with weak or strong suspect information (b = .19; p < .01). The only other variables which display a statistically significant effect on time spent are knowledge of the suspect’s vehicle plate number (b = .19; p < .01) and physical evidence (b = .09; p < .05). When a plate number was known or when physical evidence was present, more time was likely spent on the investigation. On the basis of these analyses, the following hypotheses are supported: Hypotheses 7: Cases with a higher value of stolen property are likely to have more time spent on the investigation than cases with lesser value. Hypothesis 9b: Cases with suspect information of moderate strength are likely to have more time spent on the investigation than cases with weak or strong suspect information. Hypothesis 9c: Cases with physical evidence are likely to have more time spent on the investigation than cases without physical evidence. Hypothesis 9e: Cases with a suspect vehicle plate known are likely to have more time spent on the investigation than cases without a vehicle plate known. 108 Time Allocation in Robbery Investigations In Table 8, descriptive data are presented on the independent variables and dependent variable for the robberies which were selected for a follow-up investigation (N = 292). Also reflected in Table 8 is the coding scheme used in the bivariate and multivariate analyses. Because bank robberies are unique from other robberies in that they often involve very large sums of money and necessitate the involvement of other law enforcement agencies (i.e., F.B.I.), it was of interest to explore the impact of bank robberies (N=9) on the overall distribution of scores. To do so, analyses were conducted on two sets of robbery investigations: robberies with bank robberies included and robberies with bank robberies excluded. It is apparent in Table 8 that the nine bank robberies affect the distribution of scores in two important, but not surprising, ways: value of stolen property and the amount of time spent. in the investigation. First, in the inclusive robbery category, the value loss ranges from zero to $9,132 with a mean value loss of $260. With the exclusion of bank robberies, the loss ranges from zero to $4,000 and the mean amount of loss drops to $173.5 Second, when bank robberies are included, the mean amount of time spent on robbery follow-up investigations is 4.5 hours with a range of .4 to 50.8 hours. When bank robberies are excluded, the mean amount of time spent on an investigation decreases 109 TABLE 8 INDEPENDENT AND DEPENDENT VARIABLES: # VALUES AND DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS BY "SELECTED" ROBBERIES Variable Value Robberies Robberies w/bank w/o bank N % N % Victim Type OIIndividual 189 65 189 67 1=Business 103 35 94 33 Victim Sex 0=Male 121 65 121 65 1=Female 66 35 66 35 Victim Race 0=White 132 71 132 71 1=Non-white 54 29 54 29 Victim Employment 0=Not employed 77 50 77 50 Status 1=Employed 78 50 78 60 Victim Age N= 186 186 (in years) I: 31 31 SD= 12.4 12.4 Min/Max: 12-75 12-75 Victim Income N: 145 145 (S/year) X5 14,940 .14,940 SD= 5,163 5,163 Min/Max: 7,260-37,238 7,260-37,238 Victim Desires 0=No 64 24 64 25 Effort 1=Yes 200 76 191 75 Victim- Offender Relationship 0=No 260 90 251 89 Present 1=Yes 30 10 30 11 Strength of 0=Weak/Strong 103 35 102 36 Suspect Info 1=Moderate 189 65 181 64 Physical 0=No 258 78 256 90 Evidence 1=Yes 34 12 27 10 Suspect Vehicle 0=No 249 85 242 86 Described 1=Yes 43 15 41 14 Suspect Vehicle 0=No 275 94 266 94 Lic Plate Known 1=Yes 17 6 17 6 Stolen Property 0=No 285 98 278 98 Identifiable 1=Yes 7 2 5 2 Weapon Used 0=No 109 37 108 38 1=Yes 182 63 174 62 Degree of 0=No injury 191 66 182 65 Injury 1=Minor Injury 58 20 58 21 2=Serious Injury 40 14 40 14 Value of Stolen N8 278 272 Property X: 260 173 (in dollars) SD= 855 387 Min/Max= 0-9,132 0-4,000 Time spent N: 279 270 on Follow-up Y: 4.5 3.7 Investigation SD= 5.5 2.9 (in hours) Min/Max: . -50.8 .4-16.0 # missing data are excluded from table 6 to 3.7 hours with a range of .4 to 16.0 hours. As for the distribution of the values for other variables (as presented within the inclusive category), it is seen that in the majority of cases, 121 of 187 (65%), the victims were male. In 132 of 186 cases (71%) the victims were white. Seventy-seven of 155 cases (50%) involved victims who were employed. The mean age of robbery victims was 31 years and their mean "income” was $14,940. In 64 of 264 cases (24%) the victim did not desire investigative effort. In 30 of 290 cases (10%) the victim had some sort of relationship with the suspected offender. Most of the cases, 189 of the 292 (65%), contained moderate suspect information while 103 of the 292 cases (35%) contained weak or strong suspect information. Specifically, 68 of the 292 cases (23%) contained weak information while in 35 of 292 cases (12%) the information was strong. Physical evidence was available in 34 of the 292 cases (12%). In 43 of the 292 cases (15%) a suspect’s vehicle description was available and in 17 of the 292 cases (6%) a suspect’s vehicle license plate was known. In only 7 of the 292 cases (2%) was stolen property identifiable. Weapons were used in 182 of 291 cases (63%). The majority of cases, 191 of 289 (66%), involved no injury to the victim; 58 of 289 (20%) involved minor injury; and 40 of 289 (14%) involved serious injury. As with burglary investigations, it was of interest to 111 highlight the activities performed most consistently in robbery follow-up investigations. Table 9 contains these findings. As seen, the most common activity performed was interviewing victims. In 245 of the 292 cases (84%), the victim was interviewed at least once. The second most common activity was interviewing others; in 106 of the 292 . -TI cases (36%) this activity was performed. In 82 of the 292 cases (28%) a photo line-up was conducted -- the third most 1‘]- common activity. The remainder of activities, rank ordered in frequency, are: searched computer files (26%), interviewed witnesses (23%), consulted prosecutor (23%), interviewed suspect (19%), showed mug shot books (19%), canvassed for witnesses (14%), searched crime scene (11%), interviewed informants (11%), submitted physical evidence (10%), and conducted physical line-up (10%). Table 10 contains the correlation coefficients (r) between all variables for those robberies selected for a follow-up investigation. In looking for collinearity among the independent variables, it is seen that there are several potential problematic associations (.60, .44, .22) but such associations do not appear to be pervasive. Concerning the correlations between the independent variables and time spent on the follow-up investigations, it is seen that the dollar value of the stolen property has the strongest association (.62). The higher the value of the stolen property, the more likely more time was spent on the 112 TABLE 9 FREQUENCY OF DETECTIVE ACTIVITIES IN ROBBERY FOLLOW-UP INVESTIGATIONS (N=292) Investigations in Which Activity Activity was Performed N 5 Victim Interviewed 245 84 Others Interviewed 106 36 Photo Line-up Conducted 82 28 Computer Files Searched 76 26 Witness(es) Interviewed 68 23 Prosecutor Consulted 66 23 Suspect Interviewed 55 19 Mug-Shot Books Shown 54 19 Witness Canvass Conducted 40 14 Crime Scene Searched 31 ‘ 11 Informants Interviewed 31 11 Physical Evidence Submitted 30 10 Physical Line-up Conducted 30 10 113 Amuse: sHV csosx oumHm oHoHno> n NH . mfioocH EHDOH> u N coHummHumo>cH so ucomm EBHB u SH OmnHuomoO oHoHno> n HH ova BHuoH> u N humomoum :oHouN No osHe> N u NH mosmcH>m Hmonanm u OH .ueum .QEN BHDOH> u e ausnsH no seamen u NH :oHuoEHONCH uommmsm u m oomm EHuoH> n N News cameo: u eH QHnmcoHpeHom umcsmuuoIaHuoH> u N xom BHHOH> n N oHanuHunonH muuomomm u NH vacuum you ouHmoo EHuoH> u h onus EHuoH> u H cousafioo on no: pHsoo HGOHoHuumoo Havoc mchmHa a OO.H NN. NH.I NH.I HN. OO. NO. he. ON. OH.I NN. NO.I OO. eO. NO.I OO. NN. NH oo.H NO.I NO.I NN. NO.I HO.I NN. NO. NO.I OH. NO. NH. NO. OH. HO.I NO. NH OO.H NN. eO. NO.I eH.I HH.I ON.I NO. NO.I eO. eO.I OO. eO. NH.I NN.I NH OO.H NO. NO. NH.I NH.I NH.I HN. OH.I NH. «O. OH.I NO.I OH. NN.I eH oo.H NO.I OO. NN. NO. NO. NO. NN. HO.I NO.I NO.I No. NO.I NH OO.H ON. eO.I NO. eO.I eO.I HO. NH.I NO. NH. NH. HO. NH oo.H No. NH. HO.I Ho. NO. NH.I NO.I eH. NO. HO. HH OO.H HH. NH.I NH. HO. HO. NO. NH.I NO. NN. OH OO.H ee.l NO.I NH.I NO.I NH. NO.I NO.I eN. m OO.H NH.I HO. NO.I NH.I HH. OH. ON.I N oo.H No. NH. NH. NO.I NO. NN. b OO.H NO.I HH. .NO.I NH. * N OO.H eO. HO.I HH. N N OO.H HN.I OH. # v OO.H NO.I * N OO.H * N OO.H H NH NH NH eH NH NH HH OH m N N N N e N N H ANmNflzv WHHmmmmom snflaumqmms mom mmqdem¢> 0202< mfizmHUHhmmoo ZOHB¢HNMMOO OH Manda 114 investigation. Results from a cross-tabulation (not illustrated) showed that 54 of 179 cases (30%) which involved stolen property of under $100 received more than 4.5 hours of investigative time (the mean amount of time spent). Twenty—eight of 80 cases (35%) which involved losses ranging from $100 to $650 received more than 4.5 hours of time. And 21 of 33 cases (64%) which involved over $651 of stolen property received over 4.5 hours of time spent on the follow-up investigation. Five other variables had at least a slight relationship with the amount of time spent: availability of physical evidence (.47), desire for effort (.28), victim type (.25), identifiability of stolen property (.21), and suspect information (.20). In regard to suspect information, a cross-tab procedure showed that 11 of 68 robbery cases (16%) with weak information received more than 4.5 hours of investigative time. Similarly, of the 35 cases which contained strong suspect information, six (17%) received over 4.5 hours of time. However, in cases which contained moderate suspect information, 87 of 189 (46%) received more than 4.5 hours of investigative time. In addition, when suspect information is treated as three distinct categories and then correlated with the amount of time spent on the investigation (raw form), the correlation coefficient drops to .03 from .20. As with burglaries, this evidence provides at least initial support for the hypothesis that cases with moderate suspect 115 information receive more time than cases with weak or strong suspect information. More importantly, this evidence is justification for combining weak and strong suspect information into one analytic category for the multiple regression procedure. To determine the relative impact of the victim and offense variables on the amount of time spent on the investigation, and hence test the hypotheses stated in Chapter Four, OLS multiple regression was used. These results are presented in Table 11. As seen in Table 11, analyses on robberies with bank robberies included are very similar to the analyses with bank robberies excluded with one important exception -- the influence of victim type. When bank robberies are included, victim type exerts a significant impact on the amount of time spent on an investigation; ”business" robberies were more likely to receive more investigative time (b = .14; p < .01). However, when bank robberies are excluded from the analysis, the impact of victim type disappears (b = .02; p > .05). In robbery investigations, the dollar value of the stolen property once again exerts by far the most influence on the amount of time spent on an investigation; greater property loss led to the expenditure of more time even with bank robberies (which account for much of the variance in property loss) excluded (b = .50; p < .01 with bank 116 TABLE 11 MULTIPLE REGRESSION OF TIME SPENT ON ROBBERY FOLLOW-UP INVESTIGATIONS AS A FUNCTION OF VICTIM AND OFFENSE CHARACTERISTICS Standardized Beta Independent Variables Robberies Robberies w/bank w/o bank Victim Type .14** .02 Victim Sex .00 .01 Victim Race -.03 -.02 Victim Employment Status -.02 -.03 Victil Age '002 “.02 Victim Income -.02 -.01 Victim Desires Effort .16** .19** Victim-Offender Relationship .07 .06 Suspect Information .14** .15** Physical Evidence .22** .25** Vehicle Described .05 .04 Vehicle Plate Known .00 .00 Property Identifiable .03 .02 Weapon Used .04 .05 Degree of Injury .01 -.03 $ Value of Property Loss .49*3 .50** Multiple R .72 .71 2 Adjusted R .49 .48 F 18.78 17.82 Significance .00 .00 1 N 292 as p<.01 (one-tailed test) 1 the mean of each variable was substituted for missing data 117 robberies excluded and b = .49; p < .01 with bank robberies included). The availability of physical evidence also displays a significant impact on time spent; when physical evidence was available, more time was spent on the investigation (b = .22 inclusive; b = .25 exclusive; p < .01 for both). Another significant contribution is made by the victim’s desire for an investigation. If the victim did not wish an investigation, less time was spent on the investigation (b = .16 inclusive; b = .19 exclusive; p < .01 for both). The strength of suspect information also influences the amount of time spent on the case. Cases with moderate suspect information were likely to receive more time than cases with weak or strong suspect information (b = .14 inclusive; b = .15 exclusive; p < .01 for both). Finally, in terms of the variance explained by the victim and offense variables, the model did quite well accounting for 49 percent in the inclusive category and 50 percent in the exclusive category. On the basis of these analyses, the following hypotheses are supported: Hypothesis 4: Businesses are likely to have more time spent on their investigation than non- businesses (only when bank robberies are included). Hypothesis 5: Victims who desire effort are likely to have more time spent on their investigationthan victims who do not desire effort. Hypothesis 7: Cases with a higher value of stolen property are likely to have more time spent on the investigationthan cases with lesser value. 118 Hypothesis 9b: Cases with suspect information of moderate strength are likely to have more time spent on the investigation than cases with weak or strong suspect information. Hypothesis 9c: Cases with physical evidence are likely to have more time spent on the investigation than cases without physical evidence. Summary By considering the findings across the three analyses, one can identify several patterns. First, none of the victim demographic variables (i.e., age, race, sex, income, employment status) displayed significance (except, of course, victim type in robberies with bank robberies included). Second, the dollar value of the stolen property exerted an impact on all three decisions, and was the most influential in both of the time allocations decisions. Third, strength of suspect information displayed a significant influence across all three analyses. Fourth, the presence of physical evidence had a significant impact on all of the decisions. Fifth, identifiability of the stolen property did not have an effect on any of the decisions. Finally, victim desire for an investigation, the presence of a victim-offender relationship, knowledge of the suspect’s vehicle license number, and vehicle description displayed inconsistent effects across decisions. 119 Decisions as Processes Information Board / Verbal Protocol Analysis The following data were derived from the analysis of information board search patterns and the verbal protocols. The results for each investigative decision are presented separately. The Selection of Burglaries To identify the amount of information searched (depth of search) in deciding whether or not to assign burglary cases to detectives, the proportion of thirteen case information elements accessed was calculated for each alternative and then across alternatives and sergeants. In Table 12 it is seen that the mean amount of information searched across alternatives and detectives was 46 percent. There exists substantial variance among subjects with Detective Sergeant A searching, on average, 26 percent of the available information and Sergeant B searching 57 percent. An examination of the variability of search within subjects and across alternatives shows that the depth of search is generally quite similar with relatively little variation from the mean. Table 13 presents data on the importance7 of case information in the decision of whether or not to select a burglary case for a follow-up investigation. With the 120 TABLE 12 PROPORTION OF AVAILABLE INFORMATION SEARCHED IN THE BURGLARY SELECTION DECISION Detective Sergeant Case Alternatives A B C Mean 1 .31 .62 .46 .46 2 .23 .62 .38 .41 3 .23 .46 .62 .46 4 .23 .62 .69 .51 5 .31 .54 .62 .49 Mean .26 .57 .55 .46 121 TABLE 13 THE IMPORTANCE OF CASE INFORMATION IN THE BURGLARY SELECTION DECISION Detective Sergeant Case Information Victim Type Victim Sex Victim Race Victim Employment Status Victim Age Victim Address Victim-Offender Relationship Suspect Information Physical Evidence Vehicle Described Vehicle Plate Known Property Identifiable $ Value of Stolen Property I-I ONONHN 0000000) Hid OOOOOO OOOOOOO Idh‘H O¢D¢OONN NHOOOOO’ QQ-b-bw-h ”(0°0ch HH I‘m-5&0“ OIGHOOOO OGNN¢O chcccfi H th @GfiO'H-‘N NH 122 largest scores representing the most important information, it is seen that suspect information (12.5), physical evidence (11.2), and victim type (9.8) are, on average, the most important pieces of case information. These elements were generally accessed the soonest within each alternative. None of the subjects searched victim sex, race, or employment status within any of the alternatives (0.0). When analyzing the content of search in the verbal protocols, the importance of other considerations in deciding whether or not to assign cases for investigations were highlighted as well. For example: Ok, let me see here. Case number one. First, one of the considerations I would be making is how many people I’ve got available to work... (Detective Sergeant C). ... Depending on case load this case might get assigned... (Detective Sergeant B). ... Sometimes a residential ah, canvass can be done. Only if I had detectives standing around with hands in their pockets [would this case get assigned]... (Detective Sergeant B). ... Chances are that I would assign that case unless I was very short of people... (Detective Sergeant C). ... but it would also go along with whether or not we’ve got a problem in that area, got suspects that are working that area, and perhaps the type of property that is taken and if its something that is unique and we’ve got people that are hitting that type of stuff... (Detective Sergeant C). ... there is some relationship. Again, now when I see this I want to know what the relationship is, if its an ex-boyfriend, or girlfriend, whatever the situation is here that we are talking about. And now the problem, we have an individual occupied dwelling, and there is some relationship and it could be an ex-husband, or ex-wife. 123 Did in fact they now have legal standing in that residence?... (Detective Sergeant C). ... we’ve got a lead here, a vehicle. And here again, if that is something that would click with myself or any of the investigators, then it would give us something to go on... (Detective Sergeant C). ... There again, I would want to know if that’s an area where we’ve been hit hard or not... (Detective Sergeant C). _ In Table 14 the linearity scores, as calculated through w the procedure outlined in Chapter Four, are presented for each of the subjects in the decision to select a burglary case for a follow-up investigation. The mean linearity index score is .28 which reflects a high degree of linear (or compensatory) decision making (0 reflects perfect linearity and 1 represents perfect non-linearity). Although the linearity scores vary from .14 to .38 across subjects, all of the scores fall on the linear side of the decision strategy "continuum." In an analysis of the sergeants’ verbal protocols, the linear style of decision making is also apparent. Compensatory decision strategies are most often reflected when the decision maker considers and weighs a range of information elements before rendering a decision. When a sergeant uses a compensatory strategy in deciding whether or not to assign a case for an investigation, a combination of information elements is considered and the additive 124 TABLE 14 LINEARITY OF SEARCH IN THE BURGLARY SELECTION DECISION Detective Sergeant Linearity Score A .33 B .14 C .38 Mean .28 125 weight of these elements determine whether or not the case will get assigned. For example, the following two excerpts from the protocols reflect compensatory decision making styles: Case number five. Ah, look for physical evidence. Yes there is physical evidence. Do we have a suspect? Described and could be identified on the basis of an eyewitness to the crime but no name. Ok, we’ve got a good description. I’ve got good physical evidence. Stolen property is identifiable. That is enough right there probably, we’d have an investigator assigned. 1 Because the value of the property is up to $1,000, that confirms it even more (Detective Sergeant B). With case number three we really don’t have a whole lot to go on except for some physical evidence. Right now I’m kinda wondering, on this particular case, what neighborhood this might be in. So I guess somewhere 8 along the line here I’d be kinda reading that. Ok, -- Street. But it would also go along with whether or not we’ve got a problem in that area, got suspects that are working that area, and perhaps the type of property that’s taken and if its something that’s unique and we’ve got people that are hitting that type of stuff. That would make the assignment of this case more likely (Detective Sergeant C). Although the linear index scores indicate a high degree of linear decision making, this is not to imply that selection decisions are made exclusively through the use of this type of strategy. In the analysis of the verbal protocols, the use of non-compensatory strategies are evident as well. For example, the partial protocols from Detective Sergeant A: 0k, on case number one, I’m going to look at the victim type. I’m looking to see if it’s a business or an individual. It is a business and ah, looking for a suspect. Name provided, accused was seen committing the crime. Ok, ah, physical evidence? No physical evidence. Was anything taken? No serial number. Ok, I would probably assign this case simply because I have a 126 witness who saw an individual commit the crime and provided his name. If he didn’t provide a name, I wouldn’t assign the case. In this protocol it is seen that the lack of other information such as physical evidence could not distract from the weight attached to the knowledge of a suspect’s name. However, given the apparent importance of suspect information in the selection decision for this subject, it is difficult to understand the reason for the search of any other information elements. It appears that the same decision outcome would have been rendered if "suspect information" was the only dimension searched. Perhaps if other information was found to be present, the qualifier of "probably" would not have been necessary. And case number five. Individual unoccupied dwelling. Let’s find out if we have a suspect. A§_zon_onn_§ooo_11 Described and could be identified on the basis of an eyewitness to the crime but no name. Ok, let’s see if we have any physical evidence here so we can come up with an identification. Ok, yes. See if there was a vehicle involved. No vehicle. Probably not assign this case even though you have an eyewitness. Without a name its just a shot in the dark and I probably would not assign that case (emphasis added). In this excerpt, it is seen that other factors, such as the presence of physical evidence, could not compensate for the lack of a named accused and could not move the case over the ”assignable" threshold. Prioritization of Burglaries Table 15 contains data on the proportion of the fourteen 127 TABLE 15 PROPORTION OF AVAILABLE INFORMATION SEARCHED IN THE PRIORITIZATION OF BURGLARY CASES Detective Case Alternatives A B C D E Mean 1 .64 .43 .50 .43 .64 .53 2 .57 .79 .64 .43 1.00 .67 3 .50 .71 .64 .36 .79 .60 4 1.00 .86 .64 .50 1.00 .80 5 .86 .71 .57 .57 .93 .73 Mean .71 .70 .60 .49 .87 .67 128 '1 information elements searched in each alternative for each of the detectives when prioritizing burglary cases. It is seen that the mean amount of information accessed across alternatives and detectives was 67 percent. The amount of information searched ranges from, on average, 49 percent (Detective D) to 87 percent (Detective E). An examination of the variability of search within subjects and across alternatives shows that the depth of search is generally quite variable but some subjects display more search variability (Detectives A & E) than others (C & D). Table 16 contains data on the importance of case information in the prioritization of burglary cases. It is seen that the presence of a victim offender relationship (12.2), followed by suspect information (12.0), and presence of physical evidence (11.9) are, on average, the most important pieces of case information. The factors of least importance are victim race (.2), employment status (.6), and address (2.4). With these general patterns realized, it is worthwhile to highlight the variation in importance scores across individual detectives. For example, victim desire for effort received a score of 13 with Detective C and a score of 7.4 with Detective A. Knowledge of a suspect’s vehicle plate received a score of 11 with Detective B but 0.0 with Detective D. With other examples available, it is clear that there are individual differences among detectives on 129 THE IMPORTANCE OF CASE INFORMATION TABLE 16 IN THE PRIORITIZATION OF BURGLARY CASES Detective Case Information A B C D E Mean Victim Type 14.0 5.6 14.0 14.0 8.0 11.1 Victim Sex .4 1.6 0.0 0.0 5.6 1.5 Victim Race .2 0.0 0.0 0.0 .8 .2 Victim Emp. Status 1.4 0.0 0.0 0.0 1.8 .6 Victim Age 3.2 9.0 0.0 1.6 4.6 3.7 Victim Address 1.8 2.8 0.0 1.8 5.6 2.4 Victim-Off. Rel’ship 11.2 14.0 11.0 13.0 12.0 12.2 Victim Desires Effort 7.4 10.0 13.0 9.4 9.0 9.8 Suspect Information 13.0 13.0 9.0 12.0 13.2 12.0 Physical Evidence 10.6 12.0 12.0 11.0 13.8 11.9 Vehicle Described 8.6 5.8 4.6 0.0 4.6 4.7 Vehicle Plate Known 5.0 11.0 6.0 0.0 1.0 4.6 Property Identifiable 8.8 5.0 10.0 7.4 11.0 8.4 $ Value of Property 6.6 4.2 6.6 1.6 10.0 5.8 130 the degree of importance attached to case information. The verbal protocol content of search analyses illustrates the importance of other factors in the prioritization of burglaries as well. For example: The categories that you have here, A through M, all represent very important information. You could hardly work a case without knowing this information. I think the only other thing is, ah, say under H, nature and source of suspect information. I want to know more about that person because I want to take that person to the computer, to LEMS, to LEIN. I’m going to research him before I do anything. I want to know who I’m talking to. Does he have accessibility to that area? Has he committed a number of crimes? Crimes like this before? I want to know as much about him before I talk to him. An investigation is not as clear cut as a lot of people think. In terms of knowing about the victim, I would like to know how often he reported crimes in the past. I could find that out through LEMS. I’d want to know who lives at the location of the crime. Does the suspect live there? Did he used to live there? (Detective D). ... If you can’t put the case together and information doesn’t seem to come, or you’re not getting any closer, you dump it because you don’t have Lino (Detective B; emphasis added). ... Another thing to do on case one is find out how many crimes have occurred at this business [which did not desire effort]. I’d see if there was a certain trend or a certain picture here. If they had one last month and one the month before, then you find out who the insurance company is and go from there (Detective E). If its a high value loss and the victim doesn’t want to prosecute, I want to see why. I want to see if they are insured. I want to see if they are employed or not employed. I want to see where they live. And I want to see if they were an accused in a crime somewhere themselves. So I’ll usually look up their [criminal] history as soon as I see that there is a large property loss and they don’t want to prosecute (Detective A). The linearity score for each of the burglary detectives is presented in Table 17. As seen in the table, the mean 131 TABLE 17 LINEARITY OF SEARCH IN THE PRIORITIZATION OF BURGLARY CASES Detective Linearity Score .38 .25 .09 .32 MUOW> .15 Mean .24 132 linearity score for all of the detectives is .24 which again represents a high degree of linear, or compensatory, decision making. Although the index scores range from .09 to .38, all of the scores reflect extensive use of linear decision making strategies. The compensatory style is also reflected in the partial protocols provided below. In reading the protocols, it is apparent that an understanding of each case (on which decisions of priority are based) is achieved only after particular pieces of information are considered and weighed together. Hence, the meaning of each case develops only after all the information elements are ”added together" in a compensatory style. In addition, it is seen that in several of the following excerpts (as well as some of those previously presented) certain ”questionable circumstances" lead to an increased depth of search. For example: Case number four... ... What’s the value of it? $795. That’s a lot of property. Let’s see if there was any evidence available? No evidence available. Well, what does the victim want to do? Yes they want to prosecute. With that much property taken I’m kind of curious as to where they live. What side of town? What area they live in? That’s getting to be pretty high in value. -- Avenue. My first thought is that that is a lot of property taken from that area up there. There are some nice houses over there but there are also a bunch of dirt-baggy houses too. I’d be interested in what type of property was taken. If it was cash I would really question the situation. I would go down and see where this guy works. His employment status. Not employed. Now I’ve got some real questions about it. The first thing that comes to my mind is that it is an insurance rip. This is a more common situation for a male so I’m going to look at victim sex. It’s a male. I’ll look at the race. Non-white. This kind of case, the more I see the more I wont togknow before I even go talk to somebody. I want to know as much as I can because there 133 are some real unusual circumstances. I might put this case on the back burner until I see what comes up for awhile (Detective A; emphasis added). From the same detective: Case five... ... Ok, how much was it worth? $1,000. I’m assuming that the person is going to want to prosecute with that much loss. Yes, they want to prosecute. I’ll see where they work and live with that much property loss. -- Avenue. Kind of a working class neighborhood. Ok, they’re employed. That’s not too unreasonable then. How old is this person if they’ve got that kind of property to lose? 47. That is about what I would have expected... This case appears to be probably the case which would be the most time consuming. From Detective E: Ok, normally on my regular case investigations, all of the information, if it’s available, you go ahead and correlate everything together... Now another thing that I look at here is that in case four we got a male, 32 years old, non-white, not employed, and lives on -- Avenue here in Landau. -- Avenue is an area where, it was a good area years ago but now we got a lot of problems with narcotics, dope, cocaine, and so forth and so on. So associating -- Avenue with a non-white male and $795 ripped off, first thing I’m going to be looking at is a dope rip-off of some sort. (Priority was fourth of five cases.) Another example of the need for an increased depth of search is provided below: In terms of property loss... if it is a real high value single piece of property, the owner is going to have something to prove that they own it. Like a $1,000 T.V. There are not a lot of $1,000 T.V.s and they don’t come in neighborhoods where, you know, there are dirt-bag houses. If someone lives in a house that is a twenty, twenty-two, twenty-three thousand dollar house and they’ve got a $1,500 T.V. and they don’t have a receipt for it? Now I got a real problem with that, right off the bat. Now I want to see where the guy works. See what other pieces of property he has in the house that is going to show me that he’s going to spend $1,500 on a T.V. (Burglary Detective A). 134 In the analysis of the burglary detectives’ protocols, the existence of non-compensatory decision making is evident as well but only in reference to when the victim did not desire investigative effort. For example: ... and the last [dimension] I’m going to look at as far as all cases is victim desires effort. The reason that this is done is that before you get too involved in the case you want to know if your victim gives a shit. I’m going over the categories that I’ve turned over to try and determine if I want to turn over some more of these before I make a determination or if I’m ready to make or get rid of some of these cases. Ok, at this point in time, case number one, I would not follow-up. We have a business that doesn’t care. Under victim desires effort there is a no so I’d dump it at that point. This one would not be worked on (Detective B). ... What’s big to me is this right here, victim desires effort, because I could be working a case and spending five, six, seven days on it. So this is big to me. I don’t want to waste seven days on even a legitimate crime if you’re not going to get cooperation from the victim (Detective C). ... then I would have to look at victim desires effort. No. So that closes this case. That’s the determining factor for closing this case out (Detective D). ... in most cases if the victim desires no effort, basically I don’t, I go on to the next case because number one, the prosecutor will never authorize [an arrest warrant] (Detective E). The excerpt from Detective A’s protocol (below) concerning the victim’s desire for effort is unique from those above in that although it initially reflects non-compensatory decision making, it allows for the possibility of other factors (i.e., strong suspect information) to compensate: Case one... ... Victim desires effort? No. Well, that puts this one on the bottom burner real quick. Although I might still be interested in it because it might be a case that I’m working in conjunction with something 135 else. However, the most valuable piece of information here is that we’ve got a name provided, the accused was seen committing the crime. That leaves me still real interested in the case because I still may be able to convince somebody that I still want the case. So this case, I would not can it. It’s a good case, not a piece of junk. So I may go over his head or at least pressure him a little bit. With four of the five burglary detectives then, nothing could apparently compensate for the victim not desiring effort -- even the fact that the accused was named and seen committing the crime. Accordingly, with four of the five detectives, this case was assigned the lowest priority of all the cases reviewed. Prioritization of Robberies Table 18 contains data on the proportion of the sixteen information elements searched within each alternative by robbery detectives when prioritizing robbery cases. As seen in Table 18, the mean amount of information accessed across alternatives and detectives is 59 percent. The mean amount of information searched ranges from 42 percent (Detective A) to 75 percent (Detective B). An examination of the variability of search within subjects and across alternatives shows that there is more variance with the search of Detective B than with Detective A. In Table 19, data on the importance of case information in the prioritization of robbery cases is presented. As seen, victim desire for effort (12.2), suspect information 136 TABLE 18 PROPORTION OF AVAILABLE INFORMATION SEARCHED IN THE PRIORITIZATION OF ROBBERY CASES Detective Case Alternatives A B Mean 1 .13 .25 .19 2 .13 .81 .47 3 .56 .88 .72 4 .63 .94 .79 5 .63 .88 .76 Mean .42 .75 .59 137 TABLE 19 THE IMPORTANCE OF CASE INFORMATION IN THE PRIORITIZATION OF ROBBERY CASES Detective Case Information A B Mean Victim Type 0.0 16.0 8.0 Victim Sex 0.0 7.0 3.5 Victim Race 0.0 0.0 0.0 Victim Employment Status 0.0 .8 .4 Victim Age 0.0 5.0 2.5 Victim Address 0.0 6.4 3.2 Victim-Offender Relationship 16.0 7.2 11.6 Victim Desires Effort 10.8 13.6 12.2 Suspect Information 9.0 14.8 11.9 Physical Evidence 8.4 7.8 8.1 Vehicle Described 7.8 4.2 6.0 Vehicle Plate Known 4.8 5.4 5.1 Property Identifiable 6.8 6.4 6.6 Degree of Injury 5.2 8.4 6.8 Weapon Used 10.8 9.2 10.0 $ Value of Stolen Property 6.2 5.6 5.9 138 (11.9), and the presence of a victim-offender relationship (11.6) are, on average, the most important elements of case information. These elements were generally accessed the earliest in each of the alternatives. The information elements of least importance are victim race (0.0), employment status (.4), and victim age (2.5). In this exercise at least, these information elements were rarely considered in determining case priority. With these general patterns realized, it is important to call attention to the apparent individual differences which exist in the data. For example, victim type was generally one of the first information elements accessed by Detective B, while Detective A never accessed this information in any of the alternatives. Detective A never searched any of the victim demographic characteristics, while Detective B did. The content of search analyses calls attention to other factors which are considered in prioritizing robbery cases. For example: I want to know where this thing happened but that information is not provided. I want to know if there are any parallels in these cases. Maybe the same guy did several of these. That is something that I would be looking for. Then you would bunch these cases together. My biggest concern with everything that has happened over the weekend, do we have a guy who wants to go out and rob everybody or not (Detective B). Table 20 contains the linearity scores for detectives in the prioritization of robbery cases. The mean linearity score is .35 which, congruent with the other decisions, represents a high degree of linear decision making. With 139 TABLE 20 LINEARITY OF SEARCH IN THE PRIORITIZATION OF ROBBERY CASES Detective Linearity Score A .47 B .23 Mean .35 140 the two linearity scores being .47 and .23, individual differences among detectives are once again highlighted. An example of compensatory decision making is provided in the following partial protocol from Detective B. It is seen that all of the information elements considered and weighed together (especially, degree of injury, suspect information, age of victim, and sex of victim) elevate the priority given to the case. Let’s go to case number three. Victim type? See suspect information. Property identifiable? See how much it was worth? $180. Does she want to prosecute? Yes. Victim Sex? Female. I knew it. I don’t really care if its a male or female victim. I don’t really care about the race of the victim. That’s the least of my concerns. Weapon used? No. Was she hurt? Yes, serious, broken bone. Ok, so now I need to know a whole lot of information. She deserves some immediate contact. She doesn’t appear to know who the accused was in this thing and she doesn’t have a relationship or she isn’t giving up that she has one with the suspect. She wants to prosecute. .She is out some money. I would want to find out how old she is. Above 50? 69. I knew it. Seeing as to that she is older, I have a soft spot in my heart. I got some real concerns about her. Before I even read case number four I might give her a call and just find out how she is doing... Case three will receive top priority. I base that on the fact that she did receive an injury, there wasn’t a weapon, but I want to know more about the case. She might be able to identify. If she can’t identify, then she drops in priority to maybe third. Non-compensatory decision making is also evident in the protocols of the robbery detectives. As with burglaries, non-compensatory strategies were used when the victim did not desire effort. For example: Case number one would be the last case that I would work. I would call the victim, ask why he didn’t want to follow through on prosecution, and then close the case with no further action (Detective B). 141 ... so the next thing that is important in these two cases would be ah, desire for effort. I guess that would be the next thing. Ok, case number one, that case would be gone right away. Unless you see that there is an on-going problem, you might push that victim a little harder. So this one could be a real quicky (Detective A). Summary Consideration of the findings across the three analyses shows that the greatest amount of information was searched * in the prioritization of burglaries (67%), followed by the prioritization of robberies (59%), and then the selection of burglaries (46%). In all of the decisions, the offense characteristics appeared to be of more importance than victim characteristics. Suspect information was the most consistently important factor across all decisions. Victim race and employment status appeared to be the least important across all the the decisions. Regarding the extent to which detectives use linear strategies in decision making, it was found that all of the detectives (and detective sergeants), used a primarily compensatory style. The highest degree of linearity was displayed in the prioritization of burglaries, (.24), then the selection of burglaries (.28), and then the prioritization of robberies (.35). 142 Observations The following discussion is based primarily on observations of, and interviews with, burglary and robbery detectives. Detective sergeants were not systematically observed and therefore, these data do not reflect the complexities of the case selection decision. While the data derived from the observations were broad and diverse, the discussion presented here focuses specifically on describing how cases are processed by detectives. Accordingly, additional light is cast on the decisions made by detectives, how detectives make these decisions, and how these decisions determine the amount of time spent on a case. The discussion centers around Figure 2 which is an illustration of the process by which cases are interpreted and disposed of by detectives. The organizationally recognized case dispositions (means by which cases can be "cleared") are noted within the parentheses (see Chapter Three pp. 59-60 for definitions of the statuses). This illustration also represents the framework for some of the discussion presented in Chapter Six. As portrayed in Figure 2, a follow-up investigation consists of a series of decision stages. The decisions made at each of these stages reflect the meanings attached to the case and guide the conduct of the investigation. The model is a simplification, and perhaps an over-rationalization, of 143 FIGURE 2 A DECISION MAKING MODEL OF BURGLARY AND ROBBERY INVESTIGATIONS initial investigation report assigned/reviewed does this sound like a legitimate crime? yes no victim no e—"verify facts" "verify facts"—)victim no longer longer wishes to wishes to pursue/ pursue/ close close case case (VCI/VRP) J (VCI/VRP) k does this case does this case have legitimacy? lack legitimacy? yes no yes no close clAse case case (UNF/NFI) (UNF/NFI) '6 are there "good leads?" yes no exhaust good leads perpetrator identified? yes no I § close case is this case "serious" or "unusual?" QR (WPA/RPC/COP) is there enough time to keep working J! this case? \} yes 0 cultivate close case information (NFI) "leads developed?" yes 0 exhaust leads perpetrator identified? yes nr close case ‘ (WPA/RPC/COP) continue to cultivate information/ close case (NFI) 144 a very complex process but it does seem to capture much of how burglary nnd robbery detectives think, and how they go about "working" cases. The process begins when the patrol officer’s report is assigned to, and reviewed by, a detective. After reviewing the report, the first determination is whether or not the reported crime sounds legitimate. That is, is the victim’s "story" truthful? and/or has there really been an "injustice” done here? As evident in the protocols provided earlier, ”stories" told by certain types of victims, and incidents which involve certain circumstances, are viewed by detectives as questionable in their truthfulness. Most fundamentally, did a crime really occur? Is this a phony report to rip-off an insurance company? Or, say in the context of a robbery of a taxi cab driver or a gas station attendant, did the "victim” pocket the money and then claim to have been robbed? Reports are also questioned on their legitimacy if it appears that the crime resulted from illegal activities in the first place. For example, given the facts and circumstances of the incident, does it appear that the ”robbery" was really ”a drug deal gone bad?" Was it a ”payback" where ”the victim got what he deserved?" Was the ”victim,” for example, carrying money to buy drugs? Or was it money received from selling drugs? Did a "hooker rip him off?" Generally, crimes with young black male victims, or 145 crimes which occurred in a "bad” (high drug, high crime, high non-white population) part of town, especially after dark, are questioned the most in terms of their overall legitimacy. Crimes with older white females are questioned the least. By "reading between the lines” of the patrol officer’s report and considering the details of the incident, the detective formulates an idea of what noallz happened -- he I]. . gets "a feel" for the case. At this point in the investigative process, the detective can make a tentative judgement that yes, this does sound like a legitimate crime, or no, this does not sound like a legitimate crime. With either interpretation, there is a need for the detective to ”verify the facts” of the case and find out what actually happened -- to "see if the victim’s story check’s out.” In doing so, the detective’s preliminary assessment.regarding the legitimacy of the case can be confirmed or disconfirmed. This is most often accomplished by recontacting the victim and asking him to once again to tell what happened. Specifically, this recontact can have several purposes. First, it is a means by which the patrol officer’s interpretation of the incident can be verified. Second, additional information which would help in the investigation could possibly be produced. Third, by recontacting and re-interviewing the victim, the detective can "test” the victim to see if he can provide the same 146 story twice. If there are inconsistencies between the patrol report and the account provided to the detective, it may signal an untruthful victim and a report lacking in legitimacy. Fourth, a personal contact with the victim allows the detective to see the victim and judge his credibility in person. As one burglary detective stated in a protocol, "I think case four... that’s a go-down-and-see person. I wouldn’t do any of that over the telephone. I’d want to see the person in person to judge their credibility." When "judging credibility," the victim’s "body language" can be robserved when asked "the tough questions." With such observations, further evidence could be offered as to the (il-) legitimacy of the complaint. The following message, which was on a note attached to one of the follow-up investigation reports reviewed for the statistical analyses, provides an example of how detectives use the victim’s body language to give meaning to a case: ”He did fair in the interview. When I asked him if he knew who robbed him, he dropped his eyes and said no. Maybe a little payback?" Finally, as a result of the contact, it may be learned that the victim, for whatever reason, does not wish to pursue the complaint. While detectives may question the motivations of the victim for such a desire (i.e., is this a false burglary to rip off an insurance company?), the case is most often closed without much hesitation (Victim Cancels 147 Investigation; "VCI," if culprit is not known; Victim Refuses to Prosecute; ”VRP," if culprit is known). In some cases, when questionable circumstances exist, the first question the detective asks the victim during the interview is whether or not he would "be willing to go to court and testify against the the person who committed the crime." If the victim says no, it means that the victim does not desire effort and the case is closed (as either "VCI" or "VRP"). In addition, if attempts are made to contact the victim but the victim never responds, it is assumed by the detective that the victim does not wish to pursue the complaint which also allows the investigation to be quickly closed ("VCI," ”VRP"). The information produced by "verifying the facts" can contribute additional weight to the initial judgement that the report is either legitimate or lacking in legitimacy. As stated by a burglary detective in a protocol, "I think that case number four would be easy enough to either figure out and write off as a bullshit case or maybe it is a legitimate case.” If there is a strong conviction that the case is "bullshit" or ”not legitimate" after the facts are verified, then the case would most likely be closed (as Unfounded, ”UNF;" or No Further Investigation, ”NFI"). For the large proportion of cases which remain, the question becomes: ”are there ’good leads’ for an investigation?" A ”good lead" is a piece of information 148 which is likely to ”lead" directly to the culprit and accordingly, is associated with a specific investigative activity. For example, a suspect’s name provided by an eyewitness to the crime is a "good lead" and an "interview" of the suspect is the specific activity. If a robbery victim feels that she can identify the perpetrator, and the description she provided matches that of a known robber (leads), then a photo line-up would be the specific activity. If there are are fingerprints available and a named accused is known or suspected, then the prints could be submitted for a comparison, etc. If there are "good leads” and, as a result of performing the activities associated with these leads, the perpetrator was positively linked to the crime (”tied down”), the case could be closed (Warrant Pending an Arrest, ”WPA” if an adult; Refer to Probate Court, "RPC" if a juvenile; or Accused in Other Prosecution, ”COP" when identified but not charged with the present crime). If good leads were exhausted and a perpetrator was not positively identified, on if there were no good leads to begin with, then, before more time was spent on the investigation, the following questions had to be answered: (1) is this case "serious” or "unusual" in any way? and (2) is there enough time to keep working this case? A case can be defined as ”serious" or "unusual” in myriad ways. For example, the victim could have been 149 severely injured, a large amount of property may have been taken, the crime may be part of a pattern, the victim may be of high status and have "connections," the perpetrator(s) may be seen as particularly dangerous (e.g., an "Asian gang”), or the suspected perpetrator may have personally challenged the detective at some time in this or other investigation (e.g., "come back when you can arrest me but now get the fuck out of my face"). These factors are always considered in relation to the legitimacy of the crime and the circumstances surrounding the incident. Consideration of time pressure is also important in determining how much time will be spent on a given investigation (and if the case is assigned at all as seen in the earlier protocols). If the detective is assigned numerous cases on a daily basis (vs. only a few cases per week), there is less time to work on any one case and, in order to maintain control of the workload ("avoid being swamped”), there is more pressure to close cases more quickly. If there is not enough time to keep working the case, or if the case is not unusual or serious in any way, the detective might tell the victim to call if s/he finds out who committed the crime. The case would most likely then be closed (”NFI"). If a great deal of time pressure is not perceived, or the case is serious or unusual, then the task of the § (self-motivated) detective is to cultivate information, the 150 most time consuming aspect of a follow-up investigation. The importance and usefulness of cultivating information is also increased when it is believed that just ”a little bit more” information is needed to identify the culprit. When cultivating information, the activities performed do not emanate from specific pieces of information. As a result, the activities do not have as much direction as those associated with "following leads;" information which is cultivated is often of poor quality sometimes leading the detective "in the wrong direction." Detectives may be able to cultivate information by showing mug shot books, conducting neighborhood canvases, talking with parole or probation officers to see if they know of anyone matching a given description, searching the crime scene, interviewing informants, talking to detectives from other departments to see if ”anything rings a bell" with them, or "patrolling" the area of the crime and (1) making "street stops” to ask individuals if they know anything about the crime (e.g., ”Who rides the white mountain bike around here?" "Has anybody been talking about the old lady who was ripped off?") or (2) trying to locate someone who matches a given description. As a result of these activities, "good leads" may be developed. If, in exhausting these leads, a perpetrator is identified, the case would be closed ("WPA;" "RPC;" ”COP"). If a perpetrator is not identified as a result of exhausting the newly created leads, or if no leads 151 were present in the first place, activities oriented around cultivating information could continue indefinitely or cease immediately ("NFI"). The continuation of effort and the allocation of time to such a case would once again be a function of time pressure, and the seriousness or uniqueness of the crime. In conclusion, as portrayed here, the follow—up investigation resembles a filter where cases with certain combinations of characteristics get the most amount of time devoted to them and other cases with other combinations of characteristics get the least amount of time devoted to them. Given the "big picture" sketched here, decision making of detectives sometimes reflects the non-compensatory style and at other times it reflects the compensatory style. For instance, little if anything can compensate for a victim who does not wish to pursue the complaint in determining the amount of time to devote to the case. Similarly, if a case is interpreted as "bullshit," nothing can compensate to allow the case to receive more time or effort. Alternatively, the uniqueness or seriousness of the crime or the availability of time (lack of time pressure) can compensate for the lack of evidence in a case. Cases are not always closed simply because there is a lack of evidence. Evidence is the fuel of the investigation but for some cases, as described here, efforts are made to create fuel. In the next chapter, this perspective on 152 investigations, its implications, and some of the causal linkages on which it rests are discussed further. 153 Footnotes If one included those burglaries which received a "No Report Forthcoming" status, the percent of burglaries assigned to detectives would drop to 21% (317 out of 1,536). The derivative at the mean is analogous to the standardized beta weight in linear regression. This is particularly true in explaining the inconsistency in the derivative for "suspect vehicle plate known.” Suspect vehicle plate known and suspect vehicle described have a correlation of .46. This appears to be reflected in the standard error of the ”plate known” variable (SE[B] = 386.788). The inflated standard error ultimately distorts the derivative at the mean statistic (.724) in the probit analysis. The multicollinearity does not appear to affect the regression results, however. However, although common in the literature (e.g., Sommers & Baskin, 1990; Smith & Klein, 1984), it is not appropriate to rank the impact of variables based on their degree of significance (except if the variables are measured on the same scale). With consideration of the significance levels, one may reject the null hypothesis but that is all (Johnson, Johnson, & Buse, 1987). The value losses involved in the bank robberies were: $460, $926, $2,000, $2,145, $5,100, $7,576, $9,132 (two missing). The following were the total amounts of time (hours) spent in the bank robbery follow-up investigations: 6.0, 1607’ 1800’ 2000’ 2708, 3108’ 3700’ 3705, 50080 It is necessary to distinguish between ”importance" and "influence." To determine influence, the value of the dimension searched and its impact on the decision outcome must be assessed. The determination of importance, as outlined in Chapter Four, rests with simply identifying when the dimension was accessed in each alternative. Because of the conceptual and empirical differences between "importance" and "influence," any causal inferences which are made on the basis of "importance" must be interpreted with caution. For confidentiality reasons, street names are not included in the protocols. 154 CHAPTER SIX DISCUSSION Chapter Six contains a discussion based on the results of the study. The chapter is divided into five sections. In the first section, the decision outcomes of detectives are placed in the context of previous research. The second section discusses the impact of case characteristics on the decision outcomes of detectives. The third section discusses the ”black box” of the outcome oriented results -- the cognitive processes associated with detective decision making. The chapter concludes with a discussion of the study’s limitations and directions for future research. Decision Outcomes in Context Before discussing the stimuli which provoke the decision responses of detectives and the cognitive processes associated with the decisions of detectives, it is necessary to briefly compare the decision outcomes obtained in this study with those identified in previous research. This task is complicated by the fact that only one study in the 155 literature provides statistics on a comparable population of cases. For example, Bynum et al. (1982) state that 82 percent of "all property and personal crimes" ”received little or no investigative effort” (p. 315). According to Ericson (1981), 30 percent ”of all cases" that came to the attention of the detective bureau received ”one or more hours” of investigative time. Greenwood et al. (1977) report that approximately 63 percent of robberies, 36 percent of non-residential burglaries, and 30 percent of other burglaries received "at least a half hour of a detective’s time" (p. 130). Eck (1983) however, does provide comparable figures. In Table 21, the percent of burglary and robbery cases assigned to detectives along with the mean amount of time spent on the cases in the three jurisdictions studied by Eck are presented along with those same figures obtained in this study.1 In examining the burglary statistics, it is seen that in Landau the lowest percentage of cases were assigned. Perhaps for this reason, those burglary cases that were assigned, on average, received a relatively large amount of investigative time. With robberies, there was little variance across jurisdictions in the percent of cases assigned but Landau was second to DeKalb in the amount of time devoted to them. From this comparison it appears as though the screening of burglaries in Landau followed rather stringent criteria, at least in relation to those 156 TABLE 21 DECISION OUTCOMES FOR BURGLARY AND ROBBERY CASES IN ECK (1983) AND PRESENT STUDY Jurisdiction % Burg’s Hours % Robb’s Hours Assigned Spent Assigned Spent DeKalb 45.4 1.9 100.0 5.1 St. Petersburg 35.3 1.1 100.0 .9 Wichita 76.1 .8 100.0 2.4 Landau 20.6 3.7 96.0 4.5 157 jurisdictions examined by Eck. Accordingly, although devoid of case load information (number of cases assigned / number of detectives available), the time spent on cases in Landau does appear reasonable given the context of Eck’s study. Case Characteristics and Decision Outcomes According to Black’s (1976) theory of the behavior of _"‘f " law, the seriousness of a criminal incident, and thus the 1:1... 1 "amount of law" mobilized as a result of the incident, is a function of the social structural characteristics of victims (and/or offenders). To test Black’s theory, the influence of victim characteristics on the case selection and time allocation decisions of detectives was examined. Overall, the findings of this study do not lend support to Black’s theory. In the statistical analysis, neither victim sex, race, employment status, income, nor age2 had an appreciable effect on the ”amount of law" invoked by detectives in any of the three decision situations analyzed. Only the "organization” of the victim ("victim type”) appeared to affect decision responses; robberies of ”businesses” were more likely than robberies of "non-businesses" to have more time spent on the follow-up investigation but only when banks were included in the analysis. When bank robberies were excluded from the analysis, the impact of ”organization” disappeared. In addition, opposite of the expectations outlined by 158 Black, it was found that the presence of a relationship between the victim and the offender inononnog the likelihood that a burglary report would be selected for a follow-up investigation. If Black’s hypothesis on this issue would have been supported, the presence of a relationship between the victim and the offender would have doonoooog the likelihood of a report being selected. In the two time allocation decisions, this variable did not have a significant effect in either direction. In short, of the 18 hypotheses (6 variables x 3 decision situations) derived from Black’s theory which asserted a causal relationship between victim characteristics and decision outcomes, 16 were not supported, one was partially supported, and one was contradicted. Results from the information board exercise, specifically the content of search analysis, are also generally non-supportive of Black’s predictions. Although it is difficult to make causal inferences on the basis of these data, it does appear that the social structural characteristics of victims are less important than offense characteristics in the case selection and time allocation ("prioritization") decisions of detectives. In the case selection decision, of the six victim characteristics which predict decision outcomes according to Black, only one (victim type) received an "importance score" which ranked in the top half of all scores. In the prioritization of 159 burglaries and robberies, the "importance scores" for only two of the six victim characteristics (victim offender relationship and victim type) ranked in the top half of all scores. While the regression and content of search analyses indicate that the social structural characteristics of victims do not, for the most part, affect decision outcomes of detectives, the observational data, along with several of the verbal protocols presented in Chapter Five, suggest that victim characteristics do have an impact on detective decision making. However, these influences do not take the form of independent causal relationships as assumed by Black. Rather the influences result from the combination of certain characteristics. For example, in the statistical analyses it was found that victim sex and age did not have independent effects on the time allocated to an (burglary or robbery) investigation. However, observations suggest that crimes which involved older female victims3 were more likely to be viewed as legitimate, and, in robberies particularly, much more likely to be viewed as "unusual" or "serious” than crimes with younger male, younger female, or even older male victims. On the other hand, robbery and burglary cases with younger, non-white, male victims4 were more likely viewed as lacking in legitimacy or as "bullshit," than cases with older, white, female victims. However, due to the relative rarity of such victim 160 characteristic combinations in the data, it is not possible to test these observations through statistical analyses, at least with the data available here. In addition to the victim characteristic hypotheses derived from Black, this study also examined the expectation that the preferences, or wishes, of the victim affect the decision responses of detectives. In the statistical analyses, victim desire for investigative effort was found to be influential in robbery and, to a lesser (statistically non-significant) extent, burglary time allocation decisions; victims who desired effort were more likely than victims who did not desire effort to have more time spent on their investigations ("desire for effort" was not included in the case selection decision). ' The content of search analyses were generally congruent with the statistical results although once again, causal inferences are problematic with these analyses. Victim desire for effort was ranked as the most important case information element (of sixteen) in the prioritization of robberies and fifth most important (of fourteen) in the prioritization of burglaries. Based on observational data, "desire for effort" was the only victim characteristic which had a direct and independent effect on time allocation decisions. If the victim did not wish to pursue the complaint, then typically any additional investigative effort was viewed as "a waste 161 of time" by the detective because the prosecutor’s office would not issue a warrant for the arrest of the perpetrator even if s/he was identified. Closing a case due to victim preferences was a "quick and easy" way of disposing of a case (cf. Ericson, 1981). The predictive power of this variable was perhaps mitigated in the statistical analyses by the fact that even if a victim desired effort, it did not necessarily mean that time was spent on the investigation. The case could have been quickly closed for a variety of other reasons (e.g., lack of legitimacy, lack of good leads, etc.). In placing the present "victim characteristic" findings into the context of previous detective decision making research, it is seen that the findings of this study are similar to those presented by Bynum et al. (1982). Recall, these authors found that victim sex, race, age, and employment status did not (at least directly) influence detective decision making in terms of the effort expended in follow-up investigations, although victim income in burglary cases did. Support for the expectation that victim income affects detective decision making also received support from Ericson (1981) and Waegel (1981). In addition, Waegel (1981) found that the race of the victim had an impact on detective decision making. However, none of the available studies provide a discussion of the influence of other victim characteristics on detective decision making. 162 Therefore, this study Supplements those studies previously reported by providing insight into the effect (or non-effect) of victim type, victim-offender relationship, and victim desire for effort, as well as providing additional support for the previous findings that victim age, race, sex, and employment status do not directly affect detective decision making. Finally, the observational component of this study, as well as the verbal protocol analysis, further contributes to our understanding of the V influence of victim characteristics by highlighting the I complex calculus by which these causal influences are manifested in the decisions of detectives. When placing the "victim characteristic" findings of this study (as well as of the other detective decision making studies) into the larger literature on police (patrol officer) and criminal justice decision making, it appears that victim characteristics are more influential on the decisions of police (patrol officers) and other criminal justice actors than they are on the decisions of detectives. For example, Smith and Klein (1984), Smith (1987), and Black (1970) all found victim income to have a direct and independent impact on the decisions of patrol officers (decisions to arrest or file a complaint). Victim sex was also found to affect the decisions of patrol officers (Smith & Klein, 1984; Smith, 1987) and prosecutors (Williams, 1978). Smith (1987) found that the race of the 163 victim influenced the decision of patrol officers (but see Smith & Klein, 1984). Similarly, numerous studies have found that the presence of a victim-offender relationship (or a particular type of relationship) influenced the decisions of patrol officers (Black, 1971; Smith & Visher, 1981; LaFave, 1965; Friedrich, 1977; Worden & Pollitz, 1984) and prosecutors (Schmidt & Steury, 1989) in the manner predicted by Black. The inconsistencies which appear when comparing detective decision making research with this larger body of police and criminal justice decision making research allow for the possibility that the follow-up investigation represents a unique decision stage in the justice system, distinguishable from ”police decision making" and "criminal justice decision making” more broadly, in that victim characteristics generally do not have (at least independent and direct) effects on the decisions of investigators. Alternatively, one could argue that the number of null findings produced here (especially in reference to those victim characteristics identified as important by Black) raise questions about the appropriate conception of crime "seriousness," if "seriousness" can predict the decision outcomes of detectives. It may be the case, for example, that the behavior of law is not so much a function of victim characteristics as it is characteristics of the offense, specifically those indicative of harm -- value of stolen 164 property, use of a weapon, and degree of victim injury. Of the offense characteristics which reflect the actual (or potential) harm done, only the value of the stolen property was significant in the statistical analyses. In fact, of all the case characteristics included in this study, the amount of property loss was the most influential in both of the time allocation analyses. This variable also displayed a significant impact in the case selection analyses although to a lesser degree than in the other decision situations. Despite the major impact property loss displayed in the statistical analyses, the content of search analyses shows that none of the importance scores for "value of stolen property" ranked even within the top third of all scores. This inconsistency may be explained by the frequency in which detectives use compensatory strategies in making decisions. With compensatory strategies, a high value on one dimension (e.g., $15,000 of stolen property) can compensate for a low value on another (e.g., weak suspect information) regardless of the order in which the information elements are accessed. Therefore, given extensive use of compensatory strategies and the previously discussed inability of "importance scores” to reflect the values of the accessed dimensions, it is seen once again that "importance scores" based on content of search are not necessarily valid indicators of "influence." 165 The influence of the value of stolen property on the amount of time allocated to follow-up investigations was evident in the observations as well. However, as seen in several of the protocols presented in Chapter Five, when the circumstances which surrounded the incident seemed "fishy," a large amount of property loss often raised questions about the legitimacy of the crime (recall the protocol with the theft of the $1,000 TV in the ”dirt-baggy” area of the city). However, most of the the cases which involved an ”extreme" amount of stolen property involved victims who were known in the community, and known to have such property to lose. Perhaps for this reason alone were these types of cases viewed as deserving of investigative attention. Case in point: During the observation period, a burglary was reported by a well known real estate developer in the city of Landau. This individual was described by the detectives as "owning half of the city." The claim of this victim was that $10,000 in cash was taken from his residence and the likely culprit was the exterminator (the "bug man") who had access to the house the morning the cash was discovered missing. None of the detectives doubted the ability of this victim to have $10,000 in cash taken. After numerous hours of time were allocated to the investigation (including several polygraph examinations of the suspect and a discussion of the possibility of "staking-out" the accused) the investigation was terminated without an arrest. 166 If the victim would have been "Joe Nobody," the legitimacy of the incident would have most likely been questioned perhaps to the point of being "written off as bullshit." But the important point for consideration here is that "Joe Nobody" does not report a loss of ”$10,000.” Maybe $100, $500, or even $1,000, but not $10,000. One of the detectives elegantly summarized the way in which property loss affects the time allocated to cases: The police department is a political animal. Somebody that has got $25,000 has got some political touchings. So therefore, I’ve got to pay attention to that person over the person who gets robbed of his [newspaper] money. Plus the higher the money, the more likely of that going into the press which means that my captain and my chief are going to want to know what is going on here. The media is more interested in the big money losses than the small ones. While the dollar value of the property loss was influential in the statistical analysis, neither the degree of injury nor the use of a weapon were found to affect the amount of time allocated to robbery follow-up investigations (these indicators of harm were not, of course, appropriate for burglaries). In the content of search results, degree of injury and weapon use both obtained "importance scores" which ranked among the top half of all scores (seventh and fourth, of 16 respectively). The results of the statistical analyses were generally confirmed through the observations. The only time weapon use, by itself, elevated the priority of a case or led to an increased amount of time being spent on the investigation 167 was when the culprit used some "unusual" weapon like a sawed-off shotgun or a machine gun (i.e., "an Uzi") to perpetrate the act; again, a statistically infrequent occurrence and one not captured in the operationalization of the ”weapon use" concept. An individual who used such a weapon was viewed as particularly dangerous, likely to cause great harm to someone in the future and hence, one who needed to be ”dealt with." When serious injury was inflicted on a victim as a result of a robbery, it often (but not always) gave the case enhanced credibility. However, this ”enhanced credibility” did not always translate into additional time being spent on the case. For instance, the detectives told numerous stories about "victims" who caused self-inflicted injuries and then claimed that the injuries were a result of a robbery. In one memorable incident, for example, an employee of a tire store stole cash from the store and then, once the cash was secured, hit himself over the head with a tire-iron, causing unconsciousness. It was not until other employees discovered the "victim" on the floor lying unconscious in a pool of blood that a "robbery" was reported. Because of such incidents, detectives know that injuries are not necessarily ”the truth.” In addition, consideration of the person injured (e.g., old female vs. young male) and the circumstances of the event (e.g., occurred on the street at 3:00 a.m. or in the victim’s house 168 at 4:00 p.m.) add additional meaning to the case. Finally, degree of injury is not "serious” in the same way that property loss is "serious." According to the detective’s statement cited earlier, an extensive amount of property loss is serious not because of the amount of property taken per se, but rather it is serious because of who the victim may be -- not anyone can have "$25,000" taken, but anyone can be injured. In placing these offense characteristic findings into the context of previous detective decision making research, the first realization is that very little of a context exists. Bynum et al. (1982) is the only study which directly examined the impact of property loss and victim injury on detective decision making; no studies provide insight into the impact of weapon use on detective decision making. Bynum et al. did not find property loss or injury to affect the extent of effort devoted to follow-up investigations. Therefore, this study supplements Bynum et al. in finding weapon use to not affect decision making, provides additional evidence that injury has no additive effect, and also contributes strong contrary evidence as to the effect of property loss. The study also provides insight into the meanings attached to these case characteristics and thus, how these case characteristics come to affect (or not affect) the decisions of detectives. When considering these findings in relation to those of 169 the larger police decision making literature, several parallels emerge. For example, both studies which examined the impact of weapon use on patrol officer decision making (Smith G Klein, 1984; Smith, 1987) found that weapon use did not affect decisions. Similarly, four of five studies which examined the effect of victim injury on police decision making (Berke & Loseke, 1981; Smith & Klein, 1984; Worden & Pollitz, 1984; Smith, 1987) found no (direct) effect (but see Waaland & Keeley, 1985). No studies of police decision making have assessed the impact of property loss on decision making. The consistencies between this study and studies of police decision making more broadly in terms of these offense characteristics suggest that detective decision making is similar to patrol officer decision making in that direct effects of weapon use and injury do not exist. In comparing these findings to those of previous criminal justice decision making research however, several inconsistencies do emerge. For example, Adams and Cutshall (1987) found property loss to affect prosecutorial decision making. However, Schmidt and Steury (1989) found the extent of victim injury and weapon use did impact on prosecutorial decision making. Nagel (1983) did not find weapon use to affect judicial (pre-trial release) decision making. The final set of offense characteristics examined in this study consisted of evidence, or solvability factors, associated with the crime. Of the evidence variables 170 exami physi of VI prop' were deci the spe' sue eel not we 11 examined (strength of suspect information, presence of physical evidence, description of suspect vehicle, knowledge of vehicle plate number, and identifiability of stolen property) only suspect information and physical evidence were found to influence decision making in all three decision situations. If physical evidence was available, the report was more likely to be selected and have more time ‘ spent on the investigation. Further, the stronger the [ suspect information, the more likely the report was to be L selected and when there was moderate suspect information more time was spent on the investigations than when there was weak or strong suspect information. Identifiability of the stolen property did not display an effect on any of the decisions examined. The other evidence variables displayed inconsistent effects. When a suspect vehicle was described, the report was more likely to be selected. When a suspect’s vehicle plate was known, the burglary case was more likely to receive more time. In the content of search analysis, the ”importance scores" for suspect information and physical evidence consistently ranked in the top one-third of all scores. In fact, suspect information ranked either first or second in all of the decision situations. The remaining evidence variables achieved importance scores of varying strengths. The observations support the other analyses which illustrate the impact of suspect information and physical 171 evidence on decisions of investigators. However, the impact of the other evidence factors could not be isolated through the observations. In the selection decision, the strength of suspect information most often had a direct impact on decision outcomes. In fact, in reviewing the reports for the statistical analyses, it was not difficult to successfully predict those cases which were not assigned -- if a name of a suspect was not provided (or was provided only on the basis of the victim’s guess), the case was very rarely selected for an investigation. In order for physical evidence to be predictive of case selection however, generally other evidence (albeit minimal) had to be present as well. For example, a case where a name of a suspect was provided only on the basis of the victim’s guess (weak suspect information), and fingerprints were available, would have had an increased chance of selection. Given this combination of evidence it would be relatively easy for the detective to either "make" or eliminate the named suspect. Therefore, it was seen that all of the evidence weighed together (again, in a compensatory style) was more important than each evidence element considered individually. In the time allocation decisions, most "good leads" were derived from suspect information. If no leads were present (e.g., there was weak suspect information), then the detective typically did not realistically expect much chance of an arrest clearance regardless of the time spent in the 172 ._ ._-__h._ _ _,.-._ _ inVI oftI "NF F iv was the de1 At st so ti dl investigation. As a result, the investigation was most often quickly closed through some other means (i.e., "NFI”). The only exception to this, as described in Chapter Five, was when the case was viewed as "serious” or when time was available; when either of these conditions were present, the investigation may have still had considerable time devoted to it (e.g., time was spent cultivating evidence). At the other extreme, when suspect information was quite strong (i.e., the name of the accused was provided by someone who saw the accused committing the crime), little time was needed to identify the culprit -- perhaps an interview of the suspect and a visit to the prosecutor’s office. When suspect information was of moderate strength however, it was believed that there was at least a possibility that the case "could be broken" with just a bit of additional information. These cases were therefore considered more promising and consequently, time was often ”created” for them. Once again however, strength of suspect information could have been enhanced by the presence of other evidence which could affect the overall "strength of evidence” in the case and ultimately, the amount of time devoted to the case. In looking at the detective, police, and criminal justice decision making literature, it becomes apparent that evidence (however defined) has a consistent effect on decision making across all decisions in the justice system; 173 more 8 1987; Eck, 1 AccorI chaptu stren crimi this curv info inve ex de at de st re de b0 Ca: inf more evidence leads to ”more law" (e.g., Adams & Cutshall, 1987; Albonetti, 1986; Schmidt & Steury, 1989; Black, 1971; Eck, 1983; Greenwood et al., 1977; Bynum et al., 1982). Accordingly, considering the previous discussions in this chapter, it seems reasonable to conclude that evidence strength is the one factor on which decisions of all criminal justice actors turn. At the same time however, this study empirically demonstrates the seemingly unique curvilinear relationship between strength of suspect information and the time allocated to follow-up investigations. The Processes of Decision Making This is the first known study which has attempted to explicitly trace the cognitive processes associated with the decision making of detectives, or any criminal justice actor. Accordingly, this study should be considered as only a first step toward an understanding of how detectives make decisions. In the previous section of this chapter, the statistical results and observations were discussed in relation to the factors which influence the decisions of detectives. In this section, the results of the information board exercise and observations are discussed in order to cast light on the processes by which detectives transform information inputs into decision outcomes. Specifically, 174 l— _ search behaviors of detectives are discussed in terms of their linearity, depth, and content. When detectives were presented with case information and asked either to decide which cases to select for a follow-up investigation or determine which case would receive top priority and rank the rest, it was found that all of the detectives used, to a large extent, linear decision making strategies. This result is rather surprising given the expectations outlined in Chapter Two. A substantial amount of the process tracing literature has illustrated that decision tasks characterized by a high degree of complexity (i.e., the amount of information available -- number of alternatives, number of dimensions, or both) are associated with the use of non-compensatory decision strategies (Payne, 1976; Ford et al., 1989). Although task complexity was not manipulated in this study, the amount of information available in the information boards provided what would be considered a relatively complex task in the context of previous research. In the bigger picture, the observations also lead one to believe that decision making of detectives is accomplished through primarily (but not exclusively) compensatory strategies. Based on the description of the investigative process provided in Chapter Five, it was seen that there are basically five determinations made in an investigation: (1) Is the case legitimate? (2) Does the victim wish to pursue 175 the complaint? (3) Are there good leads? (4) Is the case serious or unusual? and (5) Is there enough time to keep working the case? Each of these determinations is either directly or indirectly influenced by case information. In considering these factors, and hence determining the amount of time to spend on a case, compensatory and non-compensatory strategies are used. For example, non-compensatory strategies are used when the victim does not wish to pursue the complaint or when the case is viewed as lacking in legitimacy; if the case does not have legitimacy or the victim does not wish to pursue, little time is spent on the case. Conversely, compensatory strategies are used when the case is serious; seriousness can compensate for lack of evidence (”good leads"). Availability of time (lack of time pressure) can also compensate for the lack cf evidence (or the lack of strong evidence); if time constraints are not perceived, the case, regardless of the amount of evidence or degree of seriousness, will have time allocated to it (cf. Sanders, 1977). Hence, on the basis of the observations of detectives, one is left to conclude that detective decision making involves a mix of compensatory and non-compensatory strategies, with most decisions made through the use of compensatory strategies. Regarding the amount of information searched in the information board exercise, it was seen that the detective sergeants searched, on average, less information in the 176 selection decision than the detectives in the prioritization decision. Although determination of statistical significance is problematic give the small N, a possible explanation for this finding is that the detective sergeants have a lesser burden for action. As evident in several of the protocols, the primary task of the detective sergeants [ in reviewing initial investigation reports was to look for ‘w leads which a detective could pursue. If leads were not A E..- available, then the seriousness of the case would have to be established (vis-a-vis time pressure) before the case would be assigned. While the sergeants may formulate initial impressions about the legitimacy of the complaint, it was not their task to verify its legitimacy. Therefore this information was not necessary in order to make a determination as to whether or not to assign the case. The detective assigned the case, on the other hand, not only had to determine if there were leads and the seriousness of the incident, but he also had to be concerned with information which could assist in making an overall determination of legitimacy. Perhaps the most interesting results of the search pattern and verbal protocol analysis related to the detective’s content of search. As discussed in the previous section, victim characteristics were generally considered "less important" than offense characteristics in the search for information. More importantly perhaps is the evidence 177 provided in the protocols which supports a "gestalt theory" of decision making -- with detective decision making, the sum of the information which relates to a given case is greater than each of its individual parts. As a consequence, a case takes on meaning only when the information relating to the case is considered and weighed together. This "gestalt theory" also, of course, has direct implications for the depth of search in decision making. In Chapter Five this was referred to as "the more I see, the more I want to know" phenomenon. Limitations of the Study There are several limitations to this study. First, the data used for the statistical analyses were obtained from official reports completed by patrol officers and detectives. Official records have been questioned as an accurate reflection of objective reality (Manning, 1980; Meehan, 1986). Particularly troubling for some is the validity of the self-report measure of time spent on an investigation. On the basis of the observations, it is believed that very little error in this measure was intentionally introduced by the detectives. While detectives were required to state the amount of time spent on the investigation, the information was not monitored or organizationally reported in any fashion. There was no feedback to the detectives or supervisors on the basis of 178 these statistics. Detectives were not evaluated on this information. In addition, supervisors rarely questioned the amount of time stated on the report. Therefore, it appears that there was little reason to intentionally inflate or deflate the stated amount of time spent on a case. However, the measure did contain at least two types of errors -- memory errors and calculation errors. Concerning memory errors, through the course of a shift, a detective may have worked on five or more cases, performing one activity on one case and then another activity on another case, etc. However, sometimes the case log would not be completed until the case was closed which forced the detective to recall the activities performed and the time spent on each activity. At the other extreme, where the least amount of memory errors would be reflected, the detective would record the activity and the time spent on the activity as the activity was performed. However, most of the detectives recorded the activities performed and the time spent on the activities on a daily basis, at the end of the shift. Calculation errors resulted from the difficulties associated with estimating the time spent on various activities. For example, because of the difficulties in allocating driving time to particular investigations, it was most often excluded by the detectives. An exception to this was when a detective traveled to another jurisdiction in 179 reference to a specific case. Time spent on other activities which were not attributable to individual cases (e.g., discussions with other police personnel in the department, "patrolling" high crime [”slum"] areas) were generally not included in the summary of activities or time spent on a particular case. Finally, certain activities which consumed a relatively insignificant amount of time (e.g., departmental record computer checks) were often not recorded on the case log and therefore, were not often counted in the total amount of time spent on an investigation. Second, the observations and protocol analysis made it clear that several theoretically important variables were omitted from the analysis, most important an indicator of time pressure. Although time pressure is a slippery construct to measure, for the analysis here, a dummy coded variable could have been sufficient. Given that the greatest frequency of criminal incidents occur during the summer months and that this is also when the fewest number of detectives are available (i.e., vacations), a "crime occurred" variable could have been constructed with 1 = crime reported during summer and 0 = crime reported at all other times. A third limitation of the study is that the relatively small case sample sizes did not allow for the quantitative examination of the interaction and dependency effects 180 discussed qualitatively in this chapter. However, since the sample of burglary and robbery cases used in this study represented the population of cases which were reported in a one year time period, correcting this limitation may have required performing such a study in a jurisdiction with more (or at least more varied) burglary and robbery incidents or perhaps expanding the time frame of the study from one year to three or four years. Fourth, several problems emerged from the information board and verbal protocol analysis methodology. First, as evident in several of the protocols, decisions of investigators are based on more information than what was presented in the information board. Therefore, if this information was available in the information board, thus making the decision tasks more complex, subjects may have used non-compensatory strategies more frequently than what was observed here. Second, the information board may be criticized for removing the study of decision making from the natural work environment and thus, its artificiality. However, the decision was made to use an information board rather than hypothetical initial investigation reports in order to reduce the likelihood of order effects on the search for information. Based on preliminary observations of detectives reviewing reports, it was seen that they most often began by looking at the information placed on the top of the first page and concluded with the information on the 181 bottom of the last page. Logical perhaps, but with such a pattern, a meaningful description of content, depth, and linearity of search would have been very difficult. Third and relatedly, while the cooperation received from all of the investigators was outstanding, several of the investigators seemed, at least initially, uncomfortable with the format of the board. They were used to a narrative "to find out what happened." As a result of this unfamiliar format, natural decision processes may have been inhibited. Fourth, as discussed previously, the content of search importance scores were sometimes misleading especially when compared to statistics which indicated "influence." As discussed in Chapter Five, this problem was further compounded by the frequency in which detectives used compensatory strategies. A final limitation of this study relates to its degree of external validity. Are these results generalizable to other populations? This remains unknown, an issue perhaps which should be of concern for future research. Regardless, given the 100% case selection rate for burglaries and robberies and the 100% cooperation rate for detectives and detective sergeants in the information board exercise, the results of this study would appear representative of the populations studied. 182 Directions for Future Research As a result of this study, several directions for future research have been identified. The first area which could benefit from further research is the influence of time pressure on detective decision making. In the observations as well as in several of the protocols, attention was drawn to the importance of time pressure in determining whether or not a case was selected for an investigation (or the likelihood of a case being selected) and how much time was to be spent on the investigation. Yet this research, and other criminal justice decision making research, has not provided an adequate understanding of this important constraint on decision making. Previous psychological research has examined time pressure on decision making and has generally found that time pressure affects the processes associated with decision making. However, in such studies, time pressure is easily defined (e.g., ”Subject A had 15 minutes to perform the decision task, Subject B had 5 minutes”). In the natural work context however, time constraints are more difficult to define and identify. Hence, future research could examine where time pressure, or perceptions of it, emanates from in the natural work environment, and the effects of the perceived time pressures on the outcomes and processes of investigative decision making. Second, during the observations and in the analysis of 183 information board search patterns, numerous differences between detectives were seen. Specifically, in the observations some detectives were seen as more motivated, more ambitious, and more skillful than others. In the search patterns it was seen that some considered certain information very important while others considered it irrelevant. While this study provides preliminary evidence that individual differences exist, future research could explore in detail the nature and sources of these individual differences and their effects on the outcomes and processes of investigative decision making as well as on other outcomes associated with the investigative process (e.g., how cases are closed). Third, additional research is needed on the proper measurement of evidence strength. This study made a significant step by dissecting "strength of suspect information” and demonstrating the differential impact of various elements of evidence across decision situations. With this study as a foundation, future research could. identify the effect of various combinations of evidence on investigative decision making thus constructing a more complete index of "evidence strength." Finally, since detectives base many of their decisions upon the information provided in the patrol officer’s initial investigation reports, and efforts of patrol officers are related to the amount of information collected 184 during the initial investigation (Eck, 1979), it would be useful for future research to examine the determinants of effort and the decision processes of patrol officers during initial investigations. Accordingly, a study similar to the one conducted here, except focused on patrol officers and initial investigations, would add another dimension to our understanding of the investigative process. 185 Footnotes Because important details concerning the processes by which cases were referred to the various investigation bureaus and the validity of the self-report measure of time allocation are not provided by Eck (1983), one should be cautious in drawing inferences from this table. Victim age is not included as a social structural characteristic in Black’s theory and therefore, an absence of a relationship between age and the decision responses of detectives should not be considered as additional evidence not supportive of the theory. Because age is a demographic characteristic (as is sex, race, etc.), it is included in the discussion here. 3 In the data analyzed in this study, there were three (of 292; 1%) robbenz cases which involved female victims over 50 years of age, and fourteen (of 317; 4%) burglnry cases which involved female victims over 50 years of age. Eighteen (of 292; 6%) robbery cases involved non-white, male victims under the age of 30. Ten (of 317; 3%) burglnrz cases involved non-white, male victims under 30 years of age. 186 APPENDICES APPENDIX A Initial Investigation Report Form POLICE DEPARTMENT INVESTIGATIVE OFFENSE FORM 3", OFFENSE INCIDENT CODE ATTEMPT VIOLATION LEVEL NATURE OF OF‘ENSE COMPLAINT NUMBE‘a é IDY“ Chow Ghana ‘5 ‘ DNO USme 00m Year Month Day Tum. w OFFENSE On J orre~ss UIIISun UIZIMon UIJITN UIAMN 1 ‘ ----------------- DI. ---------------- F, occuanso am I ’ DIsIrnu Drawn Elms» Gamma... I“ g , ----- II --------- ~ -------- » ------- — occunneo neromeo TO omcen Dr I Ion-9m UIzIoar-mess Bowman... 5 NUMBER DIRECTION NAME - TYPE BLDG. APT NO FLOOR '3 8 IF INTERSECTION. NAME . TYPE NAME . TYPE .1 SUBJECT ROI-E. VT-chm CP-Commnam WT-wImess OC-Ouscoveveo Came F’s-Person Seoul-no Promo LP-Lasr Person In Possessoon D w NAME (Last. Fm Mean. Suffix) SEX RACE DOB/Age IDENTIFY INTERVIEW PROSECUTE LIVES ALONE 0 cr Um UNo Du: DNo DY“ UNo [3ch D M ADDRESS mum. Duct-on Nlmc~TyO¢. 8109 Apt No. Floor) TELEPHONE IX-om , 0°C .89 ....................................................................... 1.9.02 ..................... I-.r-. ..I 0 PS Emoyer (New Mac“) Won: HIS z o D L, BUS ' ; ~O VICTIM INJURED DI I )No Imury DI2IPoone. But Umnom DIJINon-Incaoac-mmg Druncaoacnmmq UNIFIN: C D w NAME run Fm moon Sum-I SEX RACE DOB/59¢ IDENTIFY INTERVIEW pnosecme uves ALONE 9 D C? 0ch DNo DY“ Duo DY” Uwo DY" > D m ADDRESS (Mum. Duct-on. Name-Type BN9. Act No. noon TELEPHONE Ix-can 0°C .89 .......................................................................... L‘s: ..................... '-.I- D vs Emoteyer (Name, Address! Work Hrs D L9 Bus S'O NB VICTIM INJURED D.“ We WW Dawns-om But Unknm DIJINomIncopocrmu-Iq D'Wncmacnamg D:SIFaIa ROLE NAME ILuI 5“! Mnoole sunu: ADDRESS Wombat DuocrIon, Name - Yyoe Slog. Act No.) TELEPHONE Incax D C9 SEX D M RACE R Res ‘ DOC -31 ........................................ ..---------.._---------.'. D Ps 550 IDENTIFY E mrsnwew boa. Age , a D L9 ”0 DYOI DNC IDYOS DNO Emoooyp' , Bus ' p- SE1 0 D C9 I: D m RACE rm 9” I , o Doc ------------------------------ D m D vs sea IDENTIFY INTERVIEW Doe/Ag. :1 D LP NO DY" DNo DY" 0N0 6mm Bus ' SEX 2 o D or o D oc ----- I: D '5 sea IDENTIFY Imenvuew DOB/Age D LP NO DY” DNO DY“ 0N0 Em Bus I I D C? SEX 0 WT RACE a” Req ( . [3 oc ------------------------------------------ -I ----------------------- U '5 sea IDENTIFY mrsavnew DOB/Age ‘- 0 LP "0‘ UV” DNO DY” 0N0 EmoIover 8g I I u SCENE TYPE (Godot—OTHER ' VICTIM/OFFENDER RELATIONSHIP IcooeI OTHER g ASSISTING JURISDICTION W JESS WT W MAL. DESI VALUE CS: 9mm Chm Duo ‘ REPORTING OFFICER BADGE NO DATE COMPLETED TIME ,_ use OF _ ¢ 3 Asssmc omcens suesnvuson sueenvusonv JUDGEMENT Icoos. A 187 INVESTIGATIVE OFFENSE FORM NOTE 1: ll additional victims. complainants. or suoyects are present, use additional Investigative Offense Forms NOTE 2: Mandatory Inlormation: All reports must contain all Res Gestae witnesses. statements, address and what each witness can testity to. aCCused DOB. address. rights term and where evidence can be located. 'NOTE 3: Use the lolrowing abbreviations where applicable "NA" Ior Not Applicable "REF" tor Rnlused and "UNK" tor Unknown LOCATION Number DITC‘CIIOO Name - Type Bldo Apt No Floor (examples) 125 W Claremore Or. 2 H 3 100 Bllt E Michigan Ave. It the otIense occwred without a specilic address. state in the 'Number' box the block number as In the second example. (Elk-Block) ‘. It you are using a bloat number as In the second example. use the even numbered block numbers ( l 00. 200 300. etc. I to represent the even numbered srde ot the street and the odd numbered block numbers (101. 201. 301, etc.) to represent the odd numbered side 01 the street. REPORTED TO OFFICER Reters to the time the officer is investigating the complaint. 7 ATTEMPT; A crime can only be classrtied is an Attempt when an the tollowing elements exist; 1. A physical act Is committed (e.g. burgtary where ladder Is placed on window or 080 where Victim is grabbed) 2. The perpretrator's intentions are stated by the accused or indicated by physical evidence. 3. The crime Is not completed. ADDRESS; Include meaty name it the address is other than Lansing. TELEPHONE; II a telephone does not exist. print "NONE" in the space promded. It a telephone number is outside the I 51 7 ) area code. Include the area code. SEQ. NO; A unique number should be entered in the SEO. NO. boxes lor every Victim. subIect. arrestee or suspect who is listed on the terms. I 1 .2. 3.4. etc.) LIVES ALONE Place an "X" in the "yes" Do: when the offense occwed in a re5idence. the Victim lives In that rESIdence and the victim Is the only adult living in the residence. Otherwise. enter "NA". SEX M - Male SCENE TYPE F - Female 8 - Busmess ReSIcential 01 Apartment Industrial 23 Commercial Storage U - Unknown RACE: w - White 02 Duplex 24 Construction Site 8 - Black 03 Single Family 25 Manulacturing/Factory 4i - Indian (American) 04 Residential Garage 26 Undeveloped Area H - Hispanic 05 Storage Shed 27 Warehouse A . Asian 06 Yard/Lawn/Driveway Public Premise L3 - Uni-mom firm as Cemetery VICTIMIQF=ENDER RELATIONSHIP o7 Amusement/Arcade 29 Church A Suspect anc Victim are Married 08 Appliance 09 Auto Dealer/ RV Center ‘30 Park/ Playground 31 Public Building B Ex-spouses C Suspect and Victim are Romanticalty Involved Io Bar/Restaurant 32 School 0 Parent/Child Relationsmo 11 Commercial Retail Street/Parki E BrothertsuSisterts) Relationship 12 Convenience Store -———"Q P Other Family Relationsmo 13 Drug 5'0", 33 auey G Long Term Personal Acquaintance 14 FinanCIal Institutions 15 Gas Station/Garage 16 Hotel/Motel 17 Indoor Recreational 18 Jewelry Store 19 Laundromat 20 Cleaners 34 Street/Highway 35 Parking Lot 36 Parking Ramp 37 Dumpster (arson only) 38 Vehicle (arson only) 39 Other Mobile (arson only) H Short Term Personal Acquaintance J Empl0ye:.'Empl0yee K Seit-inlticzed L Police Ollicer is WOW!) M Stranger N Other i'iise space provided atter Other.) 99 Unknown . OTHER 21 Medical Faculty '— 22 Oltice/ Business ' 40 Other (use space provided alter Scene Type‘ (code) SOLVAB'U" FACTORS YES Was there suspect(s) arrested? 1 D Was ’her'.‘ a witness to the crime? 2. D Can the suscec‘. be identitied by witness” 3. C] ’ Can a suspect be named" 4. D PATROL SUPERVISORY JUDGEMENT IS a suspect described!) 5 D 1 Depanrninal Poticy Is the Susoect lino-xvi a-d'cr can he'shc be located" 6 D 2 Geographical Cmumstances Was 2'19": signiticant MO present? 7 C] 3 Inability to Locate witnesses. Victims. Suspects Was lne'e Sig-“4'58"?! S'WS'Ca' evidence 01858”? 3 D 4 Evrdence Results Not Available It the stolen property identifiable? 9 D 5 Absence From Work Is there sapnII-canr suspect vehicle description? 10 D 6 Other. (Enter code and description at other) '49 there undeveloped leaOS" 11 D Gra. °. n1 Ollcnse 12 D Val-J0 o.-er $1.000 Qpiir-zgr over 51.000 Err-3L5 armies:nc-snitalization IeOU'IE‘O \Vaannns InVOIv'Gd 188 APPENDIX B Supplemental Report Form POLICE DEPARTMENT SUPPLEMENT FORM 189 W > p- ( C K < 2 o orrsuss INCIDENT cope Vicrius we NATURE or orreuse COMPLAINT away: 0) a Status REPORTING pencsn ewes NO. on: mama True .- K 3 ASSISTING orncsns n coo: _ no: _or _ B APPENDIX C Modus Operandi Descriptor Form LI . POLICE DEPARTMENT MODUS OPERANOI DESCRIPTOR FORM POINT OF ENTRY ENTERED THROUGH — MEANS OF ENTRY — PROPERTY POINT OF EXIT EXITED THROUGH SAFE JOBS 1 From A Altar: I Physical Force 1 From A Attic 1 Allow 2 Rear 8 Vent/Arr Condition 2 Blunt Obioct 2 Rear 3 Vent/Au Condition 2 Corner: Away 3500 CDoor 3Cumnolnstrmnt 3500 C000! JWprIiooaIScene 4 Above 0 Patio/Slidmg Glass 4 Frying Tools A Above 0 Patio/Sliding Glass 5 Beneath E Fire Escape 5 Explosives s Beneath E Fire Escape 99 Unto-town F Attached Garage 0 Pour Tools 99 UAW F Attached Garage G We! 7 Bypass Instnm G Watt 5 H manor We! 99 Unknown H Interior Wall in J Window 8 Other J window )1 K Floor K Floor 1: L Root L Root '2' III Conceal-iota 99 Unknown "' 99 Unknown M Other. N Other CODE _ CODE _ CODE __ CODE __ CODE _ CODE .— SUSPECT CHARACTERISTICS INSTRUCTIONS TO VICTIM MEANS OF ATTACK - VICTIM VICTIM ABUSE A Ransaciis/Mal Deal M Disables Telephone/Alarms I Lie Down A Physical Force I TM 3 Selective in Loot N Blinds/Cunains Drawn 2 Enter Cooler/Vane B BoaterBronen Glass 2 Blinotoldod m C Neat Burglar P Brings Own Containers 3 Commit tIegaI Acts C Belt/CadrRope 3 Beuneragged z 0 Smart 5 Grab R Does Not TsiIL Motions 4 Open Sale! Cash Register 0 Vehicle s Shots Fired Q E Avo-dsiRamovos Prints S victims Vehicle Used 5 Face Wall E Error Explosive 5 Stabbed 5 F Ests/Onnirs on Premise T Cut Power wire 6 Enter Vehicle F Firearm a victim Searched < O Arson/Attempt U UnusuaI/Abnorrnat 7 Abnormal mm 6 CS Gas 7 Hit with weapon g H Matches/Candle Used v Uses Notes 99 Umm H Sin-ting Instrument 99 Unknown 5 J Smoiies w Jumps Over Gainer/Bar a Other J Threat/Verbal e Other. 5 K Multiple Participants at Lott Note Behind K Cutting Instrument . L Hostages Y Threatens to Kill L Chem-caIrPoison '5 Z Other. M Mists/Flock til 99 Unknown in N Other 3 In COOE__.___...._______ CODE___.___..__ CODE._______..________ CODE___.__ FIREARM TYPE CARRIED FIREARM IN FIREARM HANDLING CALIBER/ GAUGE HANOGUN DESCRIPTION 1 Handgun 1 Newspaper I Shownto Victim A 22 1 N-clrel/Chrome/SIainless ' 2 Simulated/ Hand In Pocket 2 Pocxet/Coat 2 Cocks or Racirs Firearm 8 25 2 Blue Steel 3 Ram Shotgun J Belt/Waistband 3 Former: at victim C 32 3 Unusual Grip! 4 Sand OII Shalom 4 Holster 4 Lays Weapon on Counteri Bar 0 38 4 Rusty 5 Other 5 Boat 5 Puts Weapon Io Victims Body E 357 5 Detecmre E 6 Hand 5 Other. F as 99 uiiiinom “(J 7 Other 6 9 mm 6 Other 5 H :10 Gauge u. J 20 Gauge K to Gauge L 12 Gauge 99 Unknown M Other CODE ._ CODE _ CODE _ __ _ __ CODE __ CODE __ _ AREA MOTIVE NUMBER OF FIRES SET FUEL SUPPLY HEAT SOURCE t Hallway 1 Abnormat Behavior 1 Oriel-7n 1 Flammable Liouids 1 Matches 2 Mechanical 2 Boredom 2 Multiple 2 Combustible Salads 2 Lighter 3 Storage 3 Cover Other Crime 3 Whole With Trailers 3 Combustible Gasses 3 Candle 4 Utility Room 4 Dmstic s Chemicals s Timing Device 5 Omen 5 Fraud 99 Unknown 5 Electricity 2 6 Hired Arsenal 5 Other 6 Smolunq Material 8 7 Protest _ 7 Cooking Steve I: a Revenge 5 Heating unit ( 99 Unknown 9 Melptpv Cocktail 9 Other. 99 Unknom 0 Other. CODE __ CODE __ CODE _ CODE _ CODE __ REPORTING omcsn iaiea To 7’ COMPLAINT NUMBER ”GIL—OF— C 190 APPENDIX D Personal Descriptor Form ' POLICE DEPARTMENT - PERSONAL DESCRIPT OR FORM 191 NAM OF DEFENSE OFFENSE DATE OFFENSE TIME LOCATION OF DEFENSE LPDCONIPLAINT NUMBER suture? acte- Os-urtmtggo SUSPch C] a.mesreerrtccuseo Un-numwn Dawns reason NAME (Lea. Fret undue. Sufi) 0.03. ADDRESS (NW. Name-Type. Bldg. Act No.) TELEPHONE Ree Roe 0mm AKA EW/Bohou Bus HAT SHIRTIBLOUSE SKIRT/DRESS COAT PAN‘I'BISLACKS SHOES El amen UouAnowt mess on W MOTH! ADDRESS DAY TELEPHONE DISPOSITION/RELEASED TD- RACE SEX AGE HEIGHT BOLD SUBJECT wane wwntta IANaIe I 0-9 107-Under tThIn ACoatune/Unt‘tornt HMmtmaIeeonm B lack F Female 2 1043 2 VeryShonte‘r-fn 2 Meaum B Bag/ClothwehEyehoIee J Nude/PM” Ilnoien UUnmoen an." autonIS‘r-S'G'I 3W CSItllAaslt KCIoeteeoIOoooeneBetI H Hiaoerttc e was 4 Meanwr-SYI A HeevylStooIty D SIoclungOverI-Ieee L M A Aden 9 29-35 5 Tel I5'IO'-B‘I') 5 OIIeee E Halo-unmet M «mm-t U Urtletotan B 33.45 6 Very Tee (IT-Over) 99 Unlmoten F Felae Bead/W 99 Unlmoeet 7 46-95 99 Um G Rumored/Scan N Other. 9 sec 99 Draw CODE ._.. CODE __ CODE __ CODE _ CODE _ CODE ._ _ _ ...... FACIAL HAIR HAIR TYPE HAIR LENGTH HAIR COLOR HAIR FIBER TEETH INoFaaalI-lae IDyeo IBaltI/Tntn Iatack Ithayy tlAiasrng ZUnehevenIStwble 2Proceeaeo 2Cret~Cut 2MISte~beny ZKrnIty 2PM 3 memos 3 Wrg/Touoee 3 Above Ear 3 Brown 3 Bushy 3 Slarned/Decayeo 9 more 4 Sheila/Frat“ 4 Below Eat A Grey/White 9 Cum 4 Gold/Silver sGoeIee 5m SCOIOILehoIh sneoIAuburn 53tratgnt SCI-oped eFulAenohu ePonyTaa GShouloerLength 95m ”Um/NA eGeoeed TFIIIBeartI/Nouueteohe Tumors/Brads 7LongerthanShouloet Tsroun/PartIyGray SOIher TNotntngUnusuel eBearoauuetaate eNothngUmwal ”Unknown salsa/Partner» 990m 99 W 99 UnImo-n 99 um 9 Other 9 Other. 9 Other 9 Other CODE_...— CODE__ CODE_ CODE_ CODE_ CODE_— EYES EYE COLOR EYE BROw EARS COMPLEXION A COMPLSIION B SPEECH I Fate I Bretan I Thin I CauIIIIoItrer I Light I PWIthe I Imoeo’mertIIStuIIera ZCroeaeo zBIue ZBuehy 2Prot~otng 2Meo‘urn 2MOIes 2AooenItAmertoanI JSungIessee 3Hezel JConnecIeo 3Earnng 3Dark 3Frecltles 3AcoehlIFWI IGlesseetolan) AGreen eNoe-thnuauaI Auras-no eAIb-no tNoIhthnuauaI 4ForetgnLanouege 5m SGrey ”Unkno-n SNoIhrngUhusuaI SReIIOeh 99Unknowh 5NolhethhueueI B Satan/Dink 9 Pitt 5 Other 99 Unknown 99 Unknom 5 Other 99 Unlmoen 7 Irregular 1 W e Other. e m e Other: 9 Nonetgm 99 Urea-town ‘ ‘ 99 Unknown 9 Other 9 Otter. CODE _._.__ CODE CODE __ CODE ____. CODE __ CODE __ CODE __ SCAR: TATOOIBIRTINARKS (Deeon‘bet DEFORMITIES AMPUTEE (Descnbe) DEXTERITY SUBJECT INJURED I Yea I trtatgnta I Ann I Yes R Fight I Notq'ury 2N0 Homes/Designs 2Hantl 2No LLeIt 2Poeslble.butum Imuavealmlt 3NarneaIIYoroa 3Ftngers 99mm 990mm 3Non-Incebeatatno a Inieah e Torso e ——F'°' s tween 5 Leg 5 Fatal __Net:lt B m 0 Foot 7 None 7 Lump ——‘ '“ B Other a Nothrng Unusual _HRMIWtIet II um Leave M 99 Unknown __Torso _:m _Fm~ 9 °'"“ _.Leg _.__Ann _Torao _Heh¢ __Leg CODE _. _ CODE __ CODE _ CODE _ REPORT WRITTEN BY: BADGE NO OTHER OFFICER IN VEH. BADGE NO DATE-TIME REPOF DESCRIPTION GIVEN BY- PAG£_.OF== CR Use Onry (PIN) D APPENDIX E Vehicle Descriptor Form POLICE DEPARTMENT VEHICLE DESCRIPTOR FORM VEHICLE IDLE STOLEN VEHCLE u Dram Dram Drum oust-05mm UtsIstoIen a Na Recovered Cltnteum a w n um 5 van-eta Van-ole Vet-eta ¢ DIFIReootteted FOR 0mm M g REGISTEIED OWNER (Lest FtrsI. Undue. Still) TELEPHOK '53 32 05. Anoncss (w. Name - Tyeet. Aer No.) STATE News 3 > INTERIOR m OEIQRAL CONDITION ”DY DAMAGE WHEELS I Boatet Seats 7 Um heme I Pane tnearoaon I Poet I Lel I Nags zlenatSeety aaereoITaoe zeta-rm 2$ae 2Rtght 2CIttomaRtrne 3 Cw 9 floor SM 3 M 3 Good 3 Front 3 Over Size ATon't ”um AWIyITu 450.”! sites awtreRIrna SEWAdded OOIher SDeoetPael ”Um SToo SNoe-thnuauat a 9 WM ' a Pleated Part! a NoDert-tage 99 Urltnown C 7 mm 99 Unmet-n 9 Other 0 - 99 Uri-tom L’ a over. E Ill 0 MODIFIED WINDOWS LIGHTS OUT LDAA ORCUIISTMCE 3 I Front t DamagedStoeWInooura e DeoauPtaoua t Lelt Front KeynlgtlenIVehthe Dunes Drama 2 Rear 2 Damaged VII-tom 1 Cute-ta 2 tht From 2 3Jaclted-Uo 3DemaoedRearwIndot~ INar-ngUmeuel 3LeItRear we" DIIIYes UIZINo I 4 NethithmeueI 4 Ttrtted ”W athtRear PaymentsCuItenI DIIIYea 0(2IN0 “I 99 Um 5 Covered _ 9 Otter. s Brakenghts ’ .5 Other I Ltcense Plate 99 Unttnotan CODE __ __ CODE __ __ CODE _ _ __ VEHICLE YEAR VEHICLE MAKE VEHICLE MODEL VEHICLE STYLE (cede) COLORS - Top Bottom .5. < g ucsuss NUMBER uceuse STATE ucensr MONTH m YEAR ucanse coma . mm Numbers IL 3 L“. ‘5’ AUTO war MOTORCYCLE FRAME NUMBER VEHICLE INSURED av IUOAA ontyt nuance commv IUDAA only) 9 NAME (Last. first. We Surllsl ADDRESS (Number. Deeceon Name ~ Type. Act No.) g. 2, YEAR MAKE stvu l vmr MOTORCYCLE rams NUMBER ADDRESS UDAA RECOVERY DISTRICT MINER DATE RECOVERED TIME RECOVERED scans we: Ic_odeL__ ‘ " PERSONAL BELONOINGS IN VEHICLE JURISDICTION WHERE STOLEN OUTSIDE JURIS COMPLAINT NO VEHICLE CONDITDN (any 4) TOW RECOVERY INFORMATION REG OWNER But was Elm W om Um Md. Deet. Dtlt Tm Dtst No Damage Uta) Other TOWED RELEASED TO omen TOIVED er. WHERE srorteo REASON Toweo DYee DNe DYee 0N0 oereuom were con oersnomrs ms doe arrears: moment cooe 2 I 3 1 MINT HUNGER 2 a REPORTING omcen BADGE no. one COMPLETED me a- K 3 ~53on orncens IR PAGE—OF.— E | CR Use Only (Ame Nurnoert 192 APPENDIX F Property Form REPORTNG OFFICER POLICE DEPARTMENT PROPERTY FORM COMPLAINT NUMBER HOW RECOVERED CODES. 193 H IOI ER" CODES 5'5"". ' R-I ”com!“ F-Found L-Located In Second Hand Store G-Gooos Purcrtaseo L-L’I or Stolen J-Recovered tor Other JUIBOCIIOR N-NCIC/LEIN C-Conltscateo P-Cornouter OvOIher IE Iotatr' 7”. CODE BRAND/MAKE NAME SPECIFIC PRODUCT ITYPE MODEL NAMEINUMBER STYLE MODEL YEAR PRODUCT IDISERIAL NOV OPERATION ID NUMBER LPD NUMBER SIZE COLOR ' LICENSE PLATE NO. YRIMO OF PLATE Dwm VALUE LOCATION OF PROPERTY WHEN STOLEN YR PURCHASED Um m0 AT VALUE ESTIMATED BY VALLE (an) INSURANCE COMPANY INSURANCE AGENT E g FINANCE COMPANY LOCATION OF PROPERTY WHEN RECOVERED WHERE STORED POW FEW (2)06 O E m. m PW LEN CHECX FOR S‘TQEN GLN KG m DESCRIPTION UYea ONO DH! DNeg. DReg UUnreg PROP CODE BRAND/MAKE NAME SPECIFIC PRODUct, TYPE MODEL NAME/NUMBER STYLE MODEL YEAR PRODUCT ID! SERIAL No OPERATION ID NUMBER LPD NUMBER SIZE COLOR UCENSE PLATE NO YR./MO OF PLATE mm VALUE LOCATION OF PROPERTY WHEN STOLEN YR PURCHASED DMens PURCHASED AT VALUE ESTIMATED BY VALUE toooeI INSURANCE COMPANY INSURANCE AGENT N I. E. FINANCE COMPANY LOCATION OF PROPERTY WHEN RECOVERED WHERE STORED I'm ECOVERED (DOE E w. m PW LEW O'IECK FOR STOLEN GLN KG REWEST DESCRIPTION DYes DNo DHII CINeg UReo UUnreg "PROP. CODE BRAND/MAKE NAME SPECI'FI'C-PRLODUCT: TYP—E MDDEL' _NAMET N‘UM—ae' R STYLE MODEL YEAR PRODUCT ID/SERIAL NOT OPERATION ID NUMBER LPD NUMBER SIZE COLOR LICENSE PLATE No. YRJMO OF PLATE ovum VALUE LOCATION OF PROPERTY wHEN STOLEN YR PURCHASED DMens m0 AT VALUE ESTIMATED BY VALLE loom) INSURANCE COMPANY INSURANCE AGENT 0 t I 5 FRANCE COMPANY LOCATION OF PROPERTY WHEN RECOVERED WHERE STORED W m GDOE IL E m m m LEN OfCX Fm STQEN GUN KG KOJEST DESCRIPTION DYea DNo DHII DNeg DReg DUrIreg F PAGE—....OF_. APPENDIX G Follow-up Investigation Case Log ' POLICE DEW CASE LDC DATE REC’D: . COMP #: DATE cm'T: ' CRIME m2: 35519150 BY= mm: l I l I I I I —.__ _—._.. -_--— —— -._. .—_—._— _——____..-— I I l l l TOTAL m: I -_ O3 Prosecutor 09 case Review 14 Simeillance 16 BrideTIce 05 Sizspoena Ser. 10 case Research 15 Report: ertmg 21 Searm Warrant 0: Cart 11 Scene Eves; .16 Meetmgs 25 Autopsy 06 MLsc/Invo t. 1 Interv1ew . 17 Arrest O7 Mlsc/O'Lhe: 13 N. canvass CONMENI'S: E'INAL STATUS: 194 APPENDIX H Case Data Coding Form 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. INVESTIGATIVE DECISION MAKING CASE DATA FORM P.D. complaint number _______ Computer ID number Crime type? 0 = BURGLARY l = ROBBERY Victim type? 0 = INDIVIDUAL 1-8 =BUSINESSES Victim sex? 0 = MALE 1 = FEMALE 8 = MIXED Victim race? 0 = WHITE 1 = NON-WHITE 8 = MIXED Victim age? 00 = MIXED 99 = MIS’G Victim address? / S of census area /_,_ _ Victim employment status? 0 = NOT 1 = EMPL’D 8: OTHER Number of witnesses to the crime? Could suspect be identified? 1 = YE Could suspect be named? 1 Could suspect be described? 1 = YE Strength of suspect information? 1-2 Was there physical evidence present? Can vehicle be described? 1 = YE Is vehicle plate # known? 1 = ES Is stolen property identifiable? 1 = YES Victim-offender relationship? 1-2-3 Was a weapon used? 1 = YES 0 = N0 Degree of injury? 1-2-3 Amount of property loss? 5 99999 Was case selected for f-u investigation? 1 = YES 0 = N0 Detective assigned case? 10-11-12...88...99 Sgt. who screened report? 1-2-3-4-5-9 During the follow-up investigation.... Number of victim interviews Number of witness(es) interviewed Number of witness canvasses Number of others interviewed Number of times crime scene searched Number of physical evidence items submitted Number of times computer searched Number of photo line-ups conducted Number of times mug pictures shown Number of suspect line-ups conducted Number of suspects (or times) interviewed Number of informants interviewed Number of times prosecutor consulted Does victim desire investigative effort? 1 = YES 0 = NO Status of the investigation 1-2-3-4-5-6-7-8 Total time spent on investigation _____ _ _ 195 APPENDIX I Information Board: Selection of Burglaries S-.m— — Jum— V...... nah —H J... xv... 0% “Th wuh o..h 0.9m 9m - I-) J-) a-) h-.. H-) 1-... 0-7 “Tr w-.. 0-... of... 5+. — (+— rxaeo r . V. 3.9a 4.3%. kn . , 34m 13. gym» 3.29 _ A3 tum“ amen} a? 2 . Ira Sdm 3m 2.? 2.3. wand band as... a .215. 2.4.5 33> Emu; 2%; 2%; If; 5. 7% .n . z o u n U m < \n ‘11 .360 r 1 .360 m a. JndU cl 1. QnUU 47 \wndU 196 AENMIIIF! .muo: popcomonm canon cofiucahomcw 0:» :« pmpaaocw 90: mg Amman: economy :mmohpps Educa>z .mcomoon mafiacfiucopwmcoo pom "ouoz Q? Q .. Q E ...: .. ..QQE Q .3 I . -, O. I . T ...: .....:__7:..__7:é:l:_ ... Emu—m::.__nQ I . . n£§A M . - . . 1 , .33, - - . - , 9.13- N _..__.._ QHHTQEQTEHER. I in: o? o? 97 cal. ,. ”mus {\Z (\7 3% :4: 4. 1:3 . (\2 £2 «:1 33w - {1m 11.8 43415:. mourn“. 1} . d W( . . all” dud“. «0W dark §ufiflm “Mum _mmmm —mmflw ”mmmm “mmeQmmmm_ .13; . :éxw s¢1§ s53» 8%dz .2 w “H— I H m a .o _ fl— . o .... < I‘ 197 APPENDIX J Information Board: Prioritization of Burglaries ( a? -..m 57:“ J ..u v. ..v. n uh H -..... 1 ..L. o -.m m -.m .w ...m a...m u -.m 480 - M 11 r 1 Wu? an? 0:? m-_.. (-... .360 "—7 '2 J- 1' I )- J $ :1 fl 3‘ h | 3‘ H I 3‘ I I :r U | L’— u. :- U C m on a [:1 I I I E] E1 JndU d J. 198 dwUU I I N .J I N a! I N b | N H I N u. I N U C N JndU 53> $1.: 3...... I [I] «.3» .... _ . .. sun“. 333 3.1:}. .... um 3:12 .b< ‘33 é? . 31.30) j£1d> 9:1?“ 0! 9w . Sig; 036 . 113m film: 3mg...» ...-.11. {43> $49.) 2 5 V. b H I .U L U u) ( .mp0: umucmmoun vudon newpcauomcw 0:» aw vmu=Ho=w no: mfi Amman: ummaamv :mmmhvvd E«90w>: .mcomdmu hvaafiucmvwmcwv pom “0802 42: - , , (525‘ “furor” . D \u . Q .6 19‘; o .300 . . , «.2 $3.0 12 ...»)— m a t i o c . so mu} MVP“. «6*. :2 my? — o7 14...”? III \Nm 00> 1.12 a? 2 _ S .33 £22 :11, item , z 322 $13 3% 2mm 3r .2 $4 «or in...“ ...II. ru 07 +02 .4. 2 \ 1. 36 . 1...... :1Si + «vvsw ‘ ‘ , 6 ”...—03¢ MV> 0m %. 02 oz o 7 o 7 Emma '11 .0 m MU) 11W . .82 0_ i J98 . 45.02 «I . , ‘ -219 e. - V o? mtw 41. .2 a? o ”and (\7 <\7 37 (\2 (\2 (\2 1&4. - . . . . . £m, ‘ 3 2:, _ 8 :13. 1 3:02 ...—45.34 LQ 8 . 3 dug W F $3» ....a e .1... a... . ii, a}; at“ w ”3 20m“, g 2......» 2.3.5 $3 $03 Ego; 49.3., a. fim «(.3 *9;qu 3.2.: + m. .> x . . . .v . a. a m4 a. 4 u. b 4 .fl , I m u m a - J 1 4 a... _ 360 I.“ ”360 199 APPENDIX K Information Board: Prioritization of Robberies ,1. - . 5...”. m. k-“ 0...”— 7-.m 2.4m J..m yd. huh HR». 1.1m :7 .-.? :, 7:? r.- .-NEE_H-..__§. qL ii Juxn—h¢:ui‘xi E: E... in... m ...“. ...... gm. mm. J8 .315 . +1.8“ film}. 06, +911; a: - I. v. .... A 1 “212 &< $32 > slum > ¢ u 55 E +- - O . h i 030 r 1 0nd .U .m .11 QnUU .2, JndU :1 El .mhmn cmucmmoum vhcon :Owqunomcw 0:» :w umvsao:w no: mw Amman: ammhpmv anachcvc EH90w>= .mcommmh huwaaflpcovflmcoo you "0902 ”4 III-...! 0 44 3. Jam. .2.: Lana— m3? it... gazmu:aa A!) . .agnaaan \J 38 so . 13B -gyaaanammnua:aa as} {.3 . ea . ._ . 3&8. 4.13 1m). ax: “in 1%«f b3}: .%HH %3; iunw urns“ (182 £14; 2.3» Sin E «19m .... - a 2 .2 x E H l‘ J n35.93— - n a a a a a fittw 34.34 dhi J 33M 43¢ 44% dark. 38.4.2 $50; sin; ii»; _ ix»; £$&> Emu; Kid; 1 .u L m .. o m < ~30 :- \n ‘5 "G SGU undo 3 ..x am 6" . 4 6““ 1 0 2 APPENDIX L Information Board Instructions for the Selection of Burglaries INSTRUCTIONS This exercise provides a simulation of the decision of whether or not to assign a burglary case to a detective for a follow-up investigation. You should imagine that you have five cases to either "CPU" or ”OPA.” Your task is to decide which case(s) you would most likely assign to a detective. Presented here are thirteen different types of information you can consider in choosing which of five cases to assign. For example, you can look at the age of the victim, the nature and amount of suspect information, the dollar value of the stolen property, etc., all of which are typically contained in the initial patrol report. Your task is to uncover and look at as much information as you need to, and then decide which of the five cases should be assigned. While reviewing and deciding upon the cases, it would be most helpful if you "think aloud." State the information you are looking at and considering. State what you are thinking as you look at the information. State any other information which would help you in making the decision. Say anything that comes to your mind. Finally, identify the cases you would assign. All responses will be anonymous. Thank you very much for your participation. 202 APPENDIX M Depth of Search: Formula and Examples Depth of Search The following formula was used in calculating depth of search: depth of search = EDA TND where: NDA the number of dimensions accessed for a given alternative TND = the total number of dimensions available to be accessed for a given alternative The following is an example of how the equation is computed (note: an "x" indicates an accessed dimension): Dimensions 1 2. fl i §. 1 x x x x x Alts 2 x x 3 x x x 4 x x x x for... Alternative 1: NDA=5; TND=5; depth of search = 1.0 Alternative 2: NDA=2; TND=5; depth of search = .4 Alternative 3: NDA=3; TND=5; depth of search = .6 Alternative 4: NDA=4; TND=5 depth of search = .8 In this example, the mean depth of search across all alternatives = .7 or 70% of information was accessed. 203 APPENDIX N Content of Search: Formula and Examples Content of Search The procedure outlines below was used to calculate "importance scores.” In assigning scores to each dimension within each alternative, the following conventions were used: TND = total number of dimensions available to be accessed for each alternative TND“ = the first dimension accessed in each alternative TND-l = the second dimension accessed in each alternative TND-2 = the third dimension accessed in each alternative, etc. A score of ”zero" indicated that the dimension was not accessed in that alternative Accordingly, in an information board with four alternatives and five dimensions, the following search pattern could result: Dimensions 1 2. .3. .4. 5. 1 0 5 4 3 0 Alts 2 0 4 5 3 2 3 0 5 4 0 O 4 1 4 5 3 2 where, for example, in Alternative 1, Dimension 2 was accessed first, 3 second, and 4 third. Dimensions 1 and 5 were not accessed. To determine the overall "importance score" for each dimension, the mean of the assigned scores was calculated. Therefore: Dimensions 1. Z 3. 5. .5. 1 0 5 4 3 0 Alts 2 0 4 5 3 2 3 0 5 4 0 0 4 1 4 5 .3. .2. X .25 4.5 4.5 2.25 1 In this example, dimensions 2 and 3 are tied as "most important," followed by dimensions 4, 5, and then 1. 204 APPENDIX 0 Linearity of Search: Formula and Examples Linearity of Search The following formula was used in calculating the degree of linearity in decision making (from Gilliland, 1990): Degree of Linearity = NA ((08 * AU) - (D8 + AU - 1)) where: NA = the number of times a standard dimension was not accessed on a given alternative that had at least one standard dimension accessed DS = the number of dimensions accessed in the standard alternative AU = the number of alternatives used in the comparison, including the standard alternative The rationale for the components of the equation is as follows: 1. The numerator gives an indication of the degree of dissimilarity between the standard and those alternatives accessed on at least one dimension of the standard. Alternatives are limited to those accessed on at least one dimension of the standard because perfect linearity can exist even when all alternatives are not accessed. The multiplicative component of the denominator gives the size of the matrix examined for linearity. The additive component of the denominator adjusts the denominator for those elements that do not add into the numerator. Specifically, the number of dimensions in the standard are excluded because they never add into the numerator. Additionally, one dimension of each alternative will never add into the numerator because each alternative must be accessed on at least one dimension to be included. 205 The following are examples of how the equation is computed (note: an ”x” indicates an accessed dimension; 0 = perfect linearity, 1 = perfect non-linearity): 1. Dimensions 1 2 3 4 5 1 x x x x x Alts 2 x x x x x 3 x x x x x 4 x x x x x NA=O; DS=5; AU=4; Linearity Score = 0 2. Dimensions 1 2 3 4 5 1 x x x x x Alts 2 x 3 x 4 x NA=12; DS=5; AU=4; Linearity Score = 1 3. Dimensions 1 2 3 4 5 1 x x x x x Alts 2 x x 3 x x 4 x x NA=9; DS=5: AU=4; Linearity Score = .75 4. Dimensions 1 2 3 4 5 1 x x x x x Alts 2 x x 3 x x 4 x x NA=9; DS=5; AU=4; Linearity Score = .75 206 LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Abelson, R.P. & Levi, A. (1985). Decision making and decision theory. In G. Lindzey & Aronson (Eds.), o : e ugthgd. (pp. 231-309). New York: Random House. Adams, K. & Cutshall, 0.8. (1987). Refusing to prosecute minor offenses: The relative influence of legal and extralegal factors. Justice Quarterly, 1, 595-609. Albonetti, C.A. (1986). Criminality, prosecutorial screening, and uncertainty: Toward a theory of discretionary decision making in felony case processings. Criminology, 21, 623-43. Baldus, D. Pulanski, C. & Woodworth, G. (1983). Comparative review of death sentences: An empirical study of the Georgia experience. ' i a w d WI. 1.4.. 661-85- Beach, L.R. & Mitchell, T.R. (1978). A contingency model for the selection of decision strategies. Ac e of W. 3.. 439-49. Berk, S.F. & Loseke, D. (1981). Handling family violence: Situational determinants of police arrest in domestic disturbances. Law and Society fieview, lfi, 314-44. 207 Black, D. (1980). Ibo nonoozg and Customs of the Polico. New York: Academic Press. ----- (1976). Iho Bohavior of Law. New York: Academic Press. ----- (1971). The social organization of arrest. Sooofiozg Low Boviow, 23, 1087-111. ----- & Reiss, A.J. (1970). Police control of juveniles. Am2rigan_éggiglgzigal_flgzigs. 35. 63-77- ----- (1968). co r a d oc'a 0 ' t' n: Ao_Qb§orxotioo_§togy. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Michigan. Bloch, P.B. & Bell, J. (1976). gonogiog_loxootigot1oo§;_1ho Rochoster System. Washington D.C.: Police Foundation. Block, R. (1974). Why notify the police: The victim’s decision to notify the police of an assault. Criginologx, 11, 555469. Brooks, W.N. & Doob, A.N. (1975). Justice and the jury. Jourgal of Social Igsgos, 31, 171-82. Burnstein, 1., Kelly, W., & Doyle, P. (1977). Societal reaction to deviants: The case of criminal defendants. Bynum, T.S., Cordner, G.W., & Greene, J.R. (1982). Victim and offense characteristics: Impact on police investigative decision making. ggiminologz, 2Q, 301-18. Clarkson, G. (1962). E2Ltf2li2_§alsgtisni_A_§inulatign_2f Trost Invogtment. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. 208 Davis. K. (1969). Disgrgtignanz_lustigs- Baton Rouge: Louisiana State University Press. deGroot, A.D. (1965). Thought ond Qhoice io Chogg. The Hague: Mouton. Doherty, M.L. (1987). Tho Effeotg of Cognitive Cogpiexity ecision Ma i Behavio . M.A. Thesis, Michigan State University. Eek. J-E- (1983). S2lxins_Q2im§§L_Ih§_lnxgsiiaatign_21 Burglary ang Robbery. Washington, D.C.: PERF ----- (1979). InxgstisaLi2n_D22isi2n_H2dgl_BsslisaLign. Washington. D.C.: PERF. Einhorn, H.J., Kleinmuntz, D.M., & Kleinmuntz, B. (1979). Linear regression and process tracing models of judgement. Egyohoiogiooi Eovigw, gg, 465-85. Ericson, R.V. (1981). ' ° ec v Woyk. Toronto: Butterworths. Ford, 8., Schmitt, N., Schechtman, 8., Hults, B.M. & Doherty, M. (1989). Process tracing methods: Contributions, problems, and neglected research questions. Erggssses. 13. 75-117. Forst, B. & Brusi, K. (1977). A theoretical and empirical analysis of the prosecutor. igurnal_2£_L§sal_§tudig§. _. 177-91. 209 Frazier, C.E., Bock, E.W., & Henretta, J.C. (1980). Pretrial release and bail decisions: The effects of legal, community, and personal variables. Cri n o , lg, 162-81. Fredrich, R.J. (1977). The impaot of Orgahigationai, Iodividoal, god Situatiohgi Eoctoyg oh Bolioe Behovioy. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Michigan. Gaines, L.K., Lewis, B. & Swanagin, R. (1983). Case screening in criminal investigations: A case study of robberies. EQli£2_§LHQi§§. Q, 22-9. Gilliland, S.W. (1990). Iigo_Qohooyointoy_1h£oynohion u c n ’ ' e av 0 ° 0 Aopyooo_. M.A. Thesis, Michigan State University. Gottfredson, M.R & Gottfredson, D.M. (1980). Dooioioh_hokihg 1 _- u_ a_ _: T -‘ owa q .~ :: .,; -.~ i;‘ 0) Qioozotioh. Cambridge: Ballinger. ----- & Hindelang, M.J. (1979a). A study of Iho fiohovioy of Las- Amgrigsn_§2§iglgsigal_flexigs. 44. 3-18- ----- (1979b). Response: Theory and research in the sociology of law. Amgzissn_§221212£isal_flsxigg. 41. 27-38. Greenberg, B. Elliot, C.A., Kraft, L.P., & Procter, H.S. (1977). of ihyogtigotivo Eienonts of Ihformotioh. Washington, D.C.: GPO. Greeno, J.G. (1976). Indefinite goals in well structured problens- Bszghglgxigsl_flsxigs..33. 479-91- 210 Greenwood, P.W., Chaiken, J.M. & Petersilia, J. (1977). Iho WWW Lexington: Doc. Heath. Grudin, J. (1980). Processes in verbal analogy solution. hogan Eoroeption and Peyformahce, g, 67-74. Hastings, T.F. (1980). Criminal investigation. In Garmire, B.L. (Ed.), Local Governmehg Bolioe Monoggmeht. Washington, D.C.: International City Management Association. Heinz, A., Heinz, J., Senderowitz, S. & Vance, M. (1976). Sentencing by parole board: An evaluation. l2!£fl§l.2£ Criminal Law and ggimihology fil, 1-31. Hembroff, L.A. (1987). The seriousness of acts and social contexts: A test of Black’s theory of the behavior of law. American Jouyhoi of Sociology, 93, 321-47. Hindelang, M.J. & Gottfredson, M. (1976). The victim’s decision not to invoke the criminal justice process, in W.F. McDonald (Ed.). Qriminsl_lustige_and_the_li£&im- Beverly Hills: Sage. Hogarth, R.M. (1974). Process tracing in clinical judgement. Bohoyioyol_§oiohoo, 12, 298-313. Holmes, M.D., Duadistel, H.C., & Farrell, R.A. (1987). Determinants of charge reductions and final dispositions in cases of burglary and robbery. u es in ri and De i ue , 21, 233-54. Isen, A.M. & Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. §ooioi_goghitioh, _, 18-31. 211 Johnson, A.C., Johnson, M.B., & Buse, R.C. (1987). EQ2Q2Q2L£i£§i_B§§i£_§£Q_AERl12Qo New York: Macmillan- Jones, C. & Aronson, E. (1974). Attribution of fault to a rape victim as a function of the respectability of the victim. o Pe o ' and So ' s o , 2o, 415-19. Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (1976). Eye fixations and cognitive processes. Cognitize_£sxohologx. 3. 441-80o Karmen. A. (1984). Qrime_Xiotim§;_An_Introd22tion_to Viotimoiogy. Pacific Grove: Brooks Cole. King, G. (1986). How not to lie with statistics: Avoiding common mistakes in quantitative political science. Am2zi2an_l222221.21.22litisal_figi§nss. 32. 667-87. Kuykendall, J. (1982). The criminal investigation process: Toward a conceptual framework. gooynol_ofi_gyigihoi Joggioo, 19, 131-45. LaFave, W. (1965). t° e 'o t T into goggogy. New York: Little, Brown and Company. Laub, J. (1981). Ecological considerations in victim reporting to the police. looghol_o£_§zimihol_lgooioo, 19, 419-30. Lipsky, M. (1980). - v ° lhgividooi ih thiio Service. New York: Russell Sage. Lundman, R., Sykes, R.E., & Clark, J.P. (1978). Police control of juveniles. logynol_o£_flooooyoh_ih_§yino_ono Delinguengx. 15. 74-91- 212 Manning. PoK- (1980). Ihe_flaz2Ls_Game;_QrganizationaI_and InI2rmaLisnal_LimiLs_2n_nnug_La!_En12£geme_t- Cambridge: MIT Press. ----- (1978). Book review of: Detective Work (by W.B. Sanders)- §22i2logz_and_fiooial_fiesearoh. 52. 654-57. ----- (1977). 'c W r ' o ' ° of Bolioing. Cambridge: MIT Press. Mastrofski, S. & Parks, R.B. (1990). Improving observational studies of police. gyinihoiogy, 28, 475-496. Meehan, A.J. (1986). Recordkeeping practices and the policing of juveniles. beon Life, 15, 70-102. Miller, F. (1970). Byogocohioh: Iho nooigioh Lo Chogge o §u§pect with o Czimo. Boston: Little, Brown, and Company. Nagel, I.H. (1983). The legal/extra-legal controversy: Judicial decisions in pretrial release. hogyono_§ooiooy Resign..11, 481-515. Newell, A. & Simon, H.A. (1972). Homoh_£yohiog_§oiyihg. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Olshavsky, R.W. (1979). Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: A replication and extension- Qrganizationsl_aehaxioz_and_flnmsn Pontozmance, 21, 300-16. Onken, J., Hastie, R., & Revelle, W. (1985). Individual differences in the use of simplification strategies in a complex decision making task. iooyool_ofi_£gooyigohhol o o : um erc 'on d f , 11, 14-27. 213 Payne, J.W. (1976). Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An information search and protocol analysis. r a atio a Be v' Porformance, 16, 366-387. ----- & Ragsdale, E.K.E. (1978). Verbal protocols and direct observation of supermarket shopping behavior: Some findings and a discussion of methods. Advances in Consumer Research, 5, 571-577. Platt-Jendrek, M. (1984). Sentence length: Interactions with race and court. Joorhai of Crimihoi Justioe, 124 567-78. Pogrebin, M. (1976). Some observations of the detective role. Journal of Police Scienoe and Admihiotraiioh, 1, 277-84. Reid, S. (1989). Criminal Law. New York: Macmillan. Sanders, W.B. (1977). Detective Work: A Study of grimihoi Investigations. New York: The Free Press. Schatzman, L. & Strauss, A.L. (1973). Field Research: Strategies for a Natugoi Sociology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Schmidt, J. & Hochstedler-Steury, E. (1989). Prosecutorial discretion in filing charges in domestic violence cases. Criminolo , 2_, 487-510. Sherman, L.W. (1980). Causes of police behavior: The current state of quantitative research. J rna Reseagch in Crime and Delinquency, 11, 69-113. 214 Skogan, W.G. & Antunes, G.E. (1979). Information, apprehension and deterrence: Exploring the limits of IQ police productivity. Jo r a o Crimi Jus , 217-41. Skolnick, J.H. (1975). Justice Without Tgial: Law Ehforcement in Democratic Society. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Smith, D.A. (1987). Police response to interpersonal violence: Defining the parameters of legal control. Social Eogceg, go, 767-782. ----- & Klein, J.R. (1984). Police control of interpersonal disputes. Socioi Probiogo, 31, 468-81. ----- & Visher, C.A. (1981). Street-level justice: Situational determinants of police arrest decisions. Sooiai Erohiegg, 22, 167-177. Spohn, C., Gruhl, J., & Welch, S. (1987). The impact of ethnicity and gender of defendants on the decision to reject or dismiss felony charges. gyinihology, 25, 175-191. Sudnow, D. (1965). Normal crimes: Sociological features of the penal code in a public defender’s office. fiooiol 2:221:25. 12. 255-76. Svenson, O. (1979). Process descriptions of decision making. WW. go, 86-112. 215 Sommers, I. & Baskin, D.R. (1990). The prescription of psychiatric medications in prison: Psychiatric versus labeling perspectives. l2§£i£§.92§£§§£ll. 1, 739-755. Thompson. LB. (1967). MW- New York: McGraw-Hill. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty. Soionoo, 185, 1124-1131. U.S. Department of Justice (1988). Boooyt_to_oho_flotion_on Erim2_and_lustioe- Washington, D.C.: Bureau of Justice Statistics. Visher, C.A. (1983). Gender, police, arrest decisions and notions of chivalry. goinihoiogy, 21, 5-28. Von Hirch, A. & Hanrahan, K.J. (1979). est 0 of Eoyoio. Cambridge: Ballinger. Waaland, P. & Keeley, S. (1985). Police decision making in wife abuse: The impact of legal and extralegal factors. Law and Human Behavior, 9, 355-65. Waegel, W.B. (1982). Patterns of police investigation of urban crimes. 0 a of o 'ce S en a d ----- (1981). Case routinization in investigative police Welch, S. & Spohn, C. (1986). Evaluating the impact of prior record on judges’ sentencing decisions: A seven city comparison. Jgstice Quarteyly, 3, 389-407. 216 Williams, K. (1978). The Roio of the Viotim ih tho Erogocmtiom oi Violent Offomgeo. Institute for Law and Social Research. Publication Number 12. Washington, D.C.: GPO. Willmer, M. (1970). Crime and lmfozmation Theory. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press. Wilson, J.Q. (1978). nves ato s° ana i F Narcoiios Agenhg. New York: Basic Books. ----- (1968). Varioties of Police Behovior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Worden, R.E. (1989). Situational and attitudinal explanations of police behavior: A theoretical reappraisal and empirical assessment. Law ohm Sooiegy mm. 23.. 667-711 . ----- & Brandl, 8.6. (1990). Protocol analysis of police decision making: Toward a theory of police behavior. Ameyicam Journal of Crimimai Justice, ii, 297-318. ----- & Pollitz, A.A. (1984). Police arrests in domestic disturbances: A further look. haw omg Sociegy Review, 18, 105-19. Wright, P. (1974). The harassed decision maker: Time pressure, distractions, and the use of evidence. lomymoi o ' d s c o o , 59, 555-61. 217 3 43 H H H "I III sill H” V" U" El'H 1293 0 unuu‘nfimm 3