A STUDY OF THE RELATEONSHlPS BE‘IWEEN FAMILY COMMUNlCATWN PATTERNS AND ADOLESCENT AUTONOMY‘ Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNWERSlTY DANEEL E. COSTELLO 1989 *' "flw7 , LIBRA Rt mHWflflIWFMLNMIflt Mb. 3 may; This is to certify that the thesis entitled STUDY OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN‘ FAMILY COMMUNICATION PATTERNS AND ADOLESCENT AUTONOMY presented by Daniel E. Costello has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for P h .D. degree inmmation I // -/,\ //a////// Date May 16 ’ ‘ ‘ 0-169 amounts av " d HOAG & SUNS’ T 800! BRIBERY INC. --' ..l ‘ LIBRARY Btu-Beg: fl ABSTRACT STUDY OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FAMILY COMMUNICATION PATTERNS AND ADOLESCENT AUTONOHY by Daniel E. Costello This study focused on the question as to whether or not there are characteristics of the communication between family members that are associated with the child's autonomy in his thoughts and actions. The respondents were 25 families in the Holt, Michigan area. Personal interviews were conducted with the mother, father, and their son, who was between 11+ and 16 years-of—age. A tape recording was made of the family mettbers discussing various topics that dealt with adolescent problems. The child's interaction with both his mother and father was analyzed. In addition each child was asked to select the one parent from whom he generally preferred to seek advice. The chosen parent's responses were then analyzed in a "primary parent" index. Each discussion was analyzed using a modified form of Bales' inter- action process-analysis categories. In the discussion, interaction was divided into statements that concerned the parents' support of the child's ideas, the child's support of his parents' ideas, and the relative con- tribution made by the child to the total family discussion. The purpose was to check the relationships between these communication Variables and other measures of the child's level of autonomy. After c“Illupletion of their discussion, each family member was asked to make judgments concerning the child's autonomy. Daniel E. Costello Pour measures of adolescent autonomy were investigated: the child's perceived influence on his parents' decisions; the child's perceived self- confidence in his own decision-making; the extent to which the parents let their child make his own decisions; and the child's claimed use of personal and mediated sources for ideas. A correlational approach was used in testing the hypotheses. Of the twelve hypotheses tested, five were partially supported. It turned out that the most consistent predictor of the child's perceived autonomy was his communication style with his primary parent. Sole reliance on the child's relationship with either his mother or father did not point up any recurrent communication patterns. 0f the remaining seven hypotheses, two were not in the predicted direction, and the other five were not supported at all. Most of these hypotheses were concerned with the percentage of the child's contribution to the family discussion, which turned out to be a very poor predictor of the child's perceived autonomy. The data indicated that the primary parent's support of the child during the discussion consistently explained more of the variability in the child's level of autonomy than did the lack of agreement shown by the child of his primary parent's ideas during the discussion. The primary parent's support of the child in a discussion explained 21 percent of the variability in the child's perceived influence on his primary parent, while the child's lack of agreement with his primary parent's ideas accounted for 18 percent of the variability. Similarly, in explaining the child's perceived self; confidence, primary parent's support of the child's ideas during the discussion Daniel E. Costello accounted for 26 percent of the variability in this behavior, while the child's lack of agreement with his primary parent's ideas explained about 19 percent. Furthermore, the primary parent's support of the child in a conver- sation explained as percent of the variability in the primary parent's judgment whether to let the child make most of his own decisons. In addition, primary parent's support of the child's ideas during the discussion accounted for 29 percent of the variability in the child's claimed use of television, magazines, and radio for ideas and about 16 percent of the variability in the child's indicated use of his mother, father, and relatives as sources of information. A STUDY OF THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN FAMILY COMMUNICATION PATTERNS AND ADOLESCENT AUTONOMY By{\ of Daniel E. Costello A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 1969 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Completion of this study involved the guidance, labor, and goodwill of a number of people. My deepest appreciation and most sincere thanks go to all of them. My mother and father, first and utmost in my thoughts, gave me the love and support so necessary for all my life's endeavors. To Dr. Verling C. Troldahl, my thesis advisor, I extend my great appreciation. Dr. Troldahl has been instrumental in guiding this study from the beginning to the very end. The rest of my Guidance Committee also lent support and assistance to the study: Dr. Erwin P. Bettinghaus, Dr. Santo F. Camilleri, Dr. Bradley S. Greenberg, and Dr. Randall P. Harrison. My friend and colleague, Mr. Duane Pettersen, was very helpful in the phases of questionnaire construction and data collection. Technical help was kindly provided by Mrs. Teresa Shen at the Mass Communication Research Bureau, University of Iowa. The bureau secretary, Miss Marcia Hamilton, was very helpful in the typing of drafts; while Mrs. Shirley Sherman was instrumental in the completion of the final manuscript. I am thankful to Dr. Malcolm S. MacLean, Jr., director of the School of Journalism, University of Iowa, who allowed me to finish this dissertation while on the job. Finally, I must emphasize that this study is a commencement of research experience made possible with the scientific training and theoretical orienta- tion provided to me by the Department of Communication during the past four years. To Dr. David K. Berlo, chairman, who provided both financial and inspirational support, I am very grateful indeed. 11 TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER I THE RESEARCH PROBLEM . . . . . . . . An Approach to Family Study A Theoretical Interpretation of the Problem HYPOtheses eeeeeeeeeeeeeeee II RESEARCHDESIGN......... General Study Design . . . . . . Sampling Procedure . . . . . . . Operationalization of Variables Data Collection . . . . . . . . III FINDINGS O O O O O O C O 0 Description of Sample . . Analytic Scheme . . . . . Tests of Hypotheses . . . IV CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . Interpretation of Findings Contributions of the Study Suggestions for Future Research BIBLIOGRAPHY O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O 0 iii 19 19 19 22 35 37 37 38 39 59 59 65 76 79 81 TABLE 1. 2. 10. ll. 12. 13. 1“. LIST OF TABLES Description of Sample on Several Social and mmographi c Attributes O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Average (mean) Rating and Variability on Parents' Support of Child . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Average (mean) Rating and Variability on Child's Support of Parents' Ideas (N=25) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Average (mean) Rating and Variability on a Child's Perceived Influence on His Parents (N=25) . . . . . . . . . Average (mean) Rating and Variability on Parents' Control of Child's Decisions (N=2S) . . . . . . . . . . . . Average (mean) Rating and Variability on Selected Mediated and Personal Source Usage of Child (N=25) . . . . . . . . . Average (mean) Rating and Variability on Newspaper and Peer usage Of Child (N=25) . O O C C . C C C C C . C C O O . Test of Hypothesis l—-Re1ationship Between Parents' Support of Child and Child's Perceived Influence on Parents Test of Hypothesis 2--Relationship Between a Child's Support of His Parents and a Child's Perceived Influence on His Parents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Test of Hypothesis 3--Relationship Between the Percentage of a Child's Contribution and a Child's Perceived Influence on His Parents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Test of Hypothesis u—-Relationship Between Parents' Support of Child and a Child's Self-Confidence . . . . . . . Test of Hypothesis 5--Relationship Between a Child's Support of His Parents and a Child's Self-Confidence . . . . Test of Hypothesis 7--Relationship Between Parents' Support of Child and Parents' Control Over Child . . . . . . . . . . Test of Hypothesis 8--Relationship Between a Child's Support of His Parents' Control Over Child . . . . . . . . . . . . iv 21 27 28 29 31 33 3H #1 #2 an #5 H7 H9 50 c- .11 TABLE 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. Test of Hypothesis 9--Re1ationship Between the Percentage of a Child's Contributions and Parents' Over Child . . . . . . . . . Control Test of Hypothesis lO--Relationship Between Parents' Support of Child and a Child's Use of Mediated Sources . . . Test of Hypothesis lO--Relationship Between Parents' Support of Child and a Child's Use of Personal Sources . . . . . . . Test of anothesis ll--Relationship Between Child's Support of Parents and a Child's Use of Personal Sources . Test of Hypothesis ll--Relationship Between Child's Support of Parents and a Child's Use of Mediated Sources . Test of'Hypothesis l2--Relationship Between the Percentage of a Child's Contribution and a Child's Use of Both Mediated and Personal Sources 51 53 SM 56 57 58 Figure 1. LIST OF FIGURES The Interrelationship of the Three Predictor Variables and One of the Four Criterion Measums O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O 1'+ Bales' System of Observational Categories . . 23 vi APPENDIX A: APPENDIX B: APPENDIX C: APPENDIX D: APPENDIX E: APPENDIX F: LIST OF APPENDICES LETTER SENT TO RESPONDENTS DEFINITIONS OF INTERACTION ANALYSIS CATEGORIES . . . DISCUSSION QUESTIONS . . . MOTHER'S QUESTIONNAIRE . . FATHER'S QUESTIONNAIRE . . TABLE 22 O O O O O O O 0 vii 86 9Q 99 102 105 CHAPTER I THE RESEARCH PROBLEM An Approach to Family Study In the study of the family, scholars from several disciplines and from a number of schools within disciplines have done and are doing research, theory-building, and counseling. Hill and Hansen (1960), Stryker (1959, 196u), and Nye and Berardo (1966) present detailed delineations of conceptual frameworks in family analysis. For purposes of this study, an interactionist approach which has contributed a great deal to an under- standing of the family will be briefly described. Kirkpatrick (l955) describes the interactionist framework as one in which the child observes the roles played by family members and incorporates these roles into his own personality structure. In the dynamic process of family members living together, there is interaction of roles. The concept of the family as a unity is a bridge between the family institution and the individual actor as a member of a family. The unique, differentiating characteristic of the interactional approach is that it is based on the action of the family resulting from the communication process. It views family behavior as an adjustive process where cues are given and individual members respond to these stimuli. The primary focus of the framework has not been with external or environmental factors as such, but with the action of the family members in constant flux. Within the internal workings of the family, the areas receiving considerable attention have been dating, mate selection, marital adjustment, parent-child relationships, and personality formation within the family context. Thus, the major concerns have been with the processes of socializa- tion of the child and development of personality. Adults define for the child the meaning of events, values, and norms. This process is illustrated by Waller and Hill: "The child comes into the world to find an interactive system of the adults of his society." (1951, p. 39). The personality in the child develops slowly over time. A certain type of personality emerges or becomes stabilized to a degree by the interactive process of defining acts of others and thus becoming aware of one's own actions. This results in a persistent or stable pattern of behavior. Burgess and Locke (1953) refer to the source of social control for the family unit as one of the major distinctions between the institutional and interactionist frameworks. In the institutional approach, the control of behavior is derived from the social structure outside of the family. In contrast, social control in the interactionist approach is viewed as stemming from mutual affection and compatibility of the family members. It may be said that the institutional approach is more community oriented while the interactionist approach is based on interpersonal relationships among family nmmbers. It should be pointed out that the framework also differs from the structural-functional approach in sociology. Under the perspective of interaction, social action comes from acting individuals who fit their respective types of action to one another through a process of interpretation. Family interaction is the collective action of such individuals. As opposed to this view, structural-functional conceptions generally lodge social action in the action of society or in some unit of society. As a research framework the interactional approach addresses itself to a study of the internal workings of the family. In the study of the internal processes of family, the focus has been on husband-wife relation- ships or parent-child interaction. These processes consist of role-playing, status relations, communication problems, decision-making stress relations and.socia1ization processes. There seems to be a scarcity of studies which view mother-father—child interaction patterns. Also, most of the family studies have dealt with testimonial descriptions of family communication furnished by its members. These studies have not attempted to analyze the nature of the communication behavior that occurred. A Theoretical Interpretation of the Problem One of the primary functions of the family is the preparation of a child to leave home. (Douvan and Adelson, 1966). The interaction of parent and child is conditioned by their mutual knowledge of the child's eventual departure. The family's task is to rehearse the child for it and to help him rehearse himself. The direction of adolescent growth is clearly toward emancipation from the family. The period begins with the child almost entirely dependent on the family, needing its say-so for what he can and cannot do, still clinging to their ideas and ideals. It ends with the child reaching into adulthood, freer to make up his mind about what he will and will not do, holding his own beliefs and values, and, if necessary, looking elsewhere than the family for love and support. One of the key concepts in the growth and development of a child is the notion of autonomy. Autonomy is particularly important at adolescence, more so than at any other time since early childhood. Above all, at adolescence, autonomy becomes important for itself; it acquires a meaning beyond the particular, concrete issues at hand. The specific issues--what time to be in at night, or to buy one's own clothes--are important not only in themselves but also because they carry such high symbolic value. .The particular freedoms the child is given are the visible part of a larger conversation between parent and child, having to do with the child's need for later independence. Adolescent autonomy focuses on behavior and decisions. What can the youngster do on his own? What decisions can he make for and by himself? To give this concept meaning we have to know its antecedent, its sources. In the case of the adolescent, these are probably to be found in parental values. What the youngster is free or unfree to do and decide probably tells us less about him than it does about his parents; it tells us about him indirectly, through what it may suggest about the family milieu and the parents' ideology of socialization. I The parental value system is a complex notion. A variety of terms have been used to describe various types of value systems; however, the terms most often used to designate the opposing poles of this continuum are autocratic and democratic. (Lewin, Lippitt, and White, 1939). In all the value systems, interpersonal communication is an essential element. - .uw \ Given that family members are involved in face-to-face communication, the question can be raised as to whether or not there are characteristics of the interaction among the family members that are associated with other behaviors and decisions of the child. Focus of Study The present study extends the analysis of interaction to include: (I) a description of the relationship between family communication patterns and those behaviors of a child which are indicative of a child's growth toward emancipation from the family; (2) a breakdown of parent-child interaction into mother-father—child interaction patterns; and (3) the actual monitoring of family communication instead of the more typical use of testimonial reports furnished by family members. A modification of the Bales interaction process analysis categories (1950) will be used to analyze the interaction structure in the family groups. Since the family is one of the oldest and most influential learning contexts, surely a more detailed study of family communication should yield valuable clues to the etiology of such typical modes of interaction. Family Communication Patterns The following interaction patterns will be analyzed, in order to determine the nature of the communication behavior that occurs between family members. 1. Relative child support by mother. 2. Relative child support by father. 3. Relative child support by primary parent. u. Relative child support by non-primary parent. 5. Relative mother support by child. 6. Relative father support by child. 7. Relative primary parent support by child. 8. Relative non-primary parent support by child. 9. Percentage of child's contribution to family discussion. Indices of Adolescent Autonomy, The following variables are somewhat indicative of the child's growth toward emancipation from the family. Child's perceived influence on parental decisions. One of the ways in which parents can encourage independence on the part of their child is by permitting the child to express himself on the daily decisions made by them. If the parents act with understanding and generally tend to support the child's ideas, the child may start to feel that his ideas are acceptable solutions. This should result in the child's perception of himself as affecting decisions made by his mother and father. Child's self-confidence in decision-making, The degree of self- confidence that a child has in his own decision-making ability seems to be crucial to his development. How 3232.3 child is of his own judgments should indirectly indicate whether he is prepared to break his dependency on his parents for making decisions. Parent control over child's decision-making, Another indicator of whether a child has achieved a certain degree of independence is when the parents perceive the child as capable of making his own decisions. The amount of control the parents exert should indirectly indicate how well a child is prepared to make decisions on his own. up... Child's information-seeking, Some parents encourage a child to explore new ideas. The more often a child exposes himself to varied ideas from many sources, the better he should become at evaluating and using in- formation. With more sources of advice open to him, the child should become less dependent upon his parents for answers to his problems. The Relation of Small Group Research to Families Although studies of the processes of interaction in small groups provide an increasingly firm basis for making generalizations about behavior in the family, gaps in our knowledge still remain. For example, much of the empirical work has been confined to ad hgg_laboratory groups, often groups of the same sex, and frequently groups of college students. Without minimizing the value of these studies, questions can be raised concerning the generalizability of these findings and calling for research replications utilizing groups of differing composition. A concept which might help bridge this inductive gap is that of group attraction or cohesiveness. The forces affecting group attraction are usually hypothesized to be a function of the degree to which members of a group find the group experience actually or potentially need-satisfying. Attraction has been associated with formal and informal group goals and activities (Schachter, 1951), with the prestige position of the group (Back, 1951), with affectional ties to group members (Festinger, Schachter, and Back, 1950), with opportunities for free emotional expression (Festinger, Pepitone, and Newcomb, 1952), and with protection against external threat (Gerard, 1953). Cartwright and Zander (1960, p. 70) point out some additional ways of viewing group attraction when they state: A cohesive group might be characterized as one in which the members all work together for a common goal, or where everyone is ready to take responsibility for group chores. The willingness to endure pain or frustration is yet another possible indication of its cohesiveness. Finally, we might conceive of a cohesive group as one which its members will defend against criticism or attack. Before the findings in small group research can be applied to family groups, an assumption must be made. The assumption is that groups high in attraction, whether ad hoc groups or family groups, have some common correlates. If this is so, the correlates of group attractiveness found in small group research should say something about the characteristics of attractive or cohesive families. Review of the Literature The democratic-authoritarian concept has been found to apply to both task-oriented small groups and family groups. Some of the characteristics that are commonly associated with democratic-authoritarian value systems should provide insights into family interaction patterns. The parents' allowance of some form of expressed conflict seems a crucial variable for the comfort and development of the adolescent child. Douvan and Adelson conducted two national studies based on interviews with some 3500 children. Their studies include both boys and girls in grades 6 through 12, with no age limits. They found that boys and girls who report .ga high rate of disagreement with their parents come from more democratic or equalitarian homes. Also, children from equalitarian homes indicate a high level of participation in family decisions. e :JJfl ' -_ -,‘VY(<’A'. 9". fl.\1'v- .. ”(I n Thus, another important variable is whether the parents give the child an opportunity to participate in decisions which concern the family. The basic question here seems to be whether the parents hold the power for all decisions affecting the child or do they permit the child a degree of self-determination? It seems that the authoritarian parent will neither explain his act nor permit the child to question it. On the other hand, the parent who assumes a rational base for power treats the child as an understanding being, explaining his own acts and responding to the child's questions and arguments. The interpretation of their results relies less on the notion of conflict than on a picture of relatively easy relationships in a family pattern where the parents encourage independence, permit the child to feel and express his differences and disagreements, and exercise moderate authority which they both explain and allow to be questioned. Sargent (1967) conducted a study to determine whether democratic and authoritarian leaders differ systematically in certain communication behaviors. He classified 4-H Club leaders as democratic or authoritarian, and then, tape recorded their discussions with club members. The communi- cation behaviors of the leaders were content-analyzed according to the major categories used by Bales. He found that authoritarian leaders made more attempts to offer answers to the discussion questions; while the democratic leaders phrased more of their contributions to the group in the form of questions. In other words, authoritarian leaders more frequently offered solutions to the problems; while democratic leaders more frequently assisted the group in finding a group solution. .e- n».- 10 Sargent also found that authoritarian leaders made more negative Social- Emotional (non-supportive) comments than did the democratic leaders. There was also a tendency for democratic leaders to make a higher ratio of positive Social-Emotional (supportive) statements. In addition, democratic leaders made more attempts to encourage participation in the discussion by group members. They offered more con- tributions to the group in the form of questions, alternatives, or Speculations. Furthermore, authoritarian leaders, according to Lippitt and White (1960), attempt to make all decisions themselves. Gibb (1951) found that democratic leadership was associated with freedom of expression. Also, riLikert (1961, p. 171) states that a democratic leadership style involves "giving the group members ample opportunity to express their thoughts with- out being constrained by the leader pressing his own views." In a democratic family system, then, one might expect parents to give a great deal of reinforcement to their children; while at the same time allowing the child to disagree with their ideas. On the other hand, an authoritarian family system might be characterized as giving the child very little support; while showing very little tolerance for expressed disagree- ment on the part of the child. This type of family system would tend to discount or limit a child's views, especially if these views were perceived as antagonistic to those of the parents. In addition, a child from a democratic family system would probably be encouraged to participate in family discussions; while a child from an authoritarian system would have little freedom to contribute his own ideas. 11 Participation and support have also been found to be related to group attraction or cohesiveness. Costley (1964) investigated the relation- ships between interaction characteristics in small group communication and sources of group attraction. The subjects were assigned to three-man groups and participated in a thirty minute discussion of civil rights issues. A modification of Bales interaction process analysis was used to content analyze the discussion. He found a negative relationship between the percentage of negative social-emotional reactions and each of the following variables: (a) personal attraction, (b) coordination of effort, and (c) satisfaction with group decisions. In addition, he found a positive relationship between frequency of interaction and the percentage of attempted answers in the task area. Cartwright and Zander (1953, p. 89), in their summary of research on group cohesiveness, indicate a number of correlates. They report that attracted members, while willing to listen to others, also try readily to influence others. In addition, attracted members are reported as usually accepting of others' opinions, and often change their minds to take the views of fellow members. These findings are similar to the previous description of a democratic family system. The democratic family pattern is where the parents encourage independence, permit the child to feel and express his differences and disagreements, and exercise moderate authority which they both explain and allow to be questioned. Thus, it would seem that a democratic family system would be defined as a cohesive group. 12 In an attempt to handle the oversimplification of the authoritarian- democratic dichotonomy, McLeod, Chaffee, and Wackman (1967, p. 2) devised a two-factor model of family types. They indicate that parents emphasize either or both of these two kinds of structural relations in raising their children. The first kind of relation is called socio-oriented, in which the child is encouraged to maintain harmonious personal relations with his parents and others. The second is called concept-oriented, in which the child is encouraged to express his ideas and to challenge others' beliefs. The division of each dimension into high and low yields a fourfold typology of family types. "Laissez-faire families emphasize neither type of relation. Children are not prohibited from challenging parental views, but neither are they exposed to information relevant to expressing independent ideas." "Protective families stress socio-relations at the expense of concept-relations. The child is encouraged to get along with others by steering clear of the controversial realm of ideas. Not only is be prohibited from expressing dissent, but he is given little chance to encounter information on which to base his own views." "Pluralistic families emphasize the development of strong and varied concept-relations in an environment comparatively free of social restraints. The child is encouraged to explore new ideas and is often eXposed to controversial material; thus, he can make up his own mind without fear that reaching a different conclusion from his parents will endanger social relations in the family." "Consensual families stress both types of relations. While the child is exposed to controversy, he is constrained to develop concepts that are consonant with the existing socio- relations--i.e., to learn his parents' ideas and to adopt their views." 13 The investigators then studied the relationship between family communi- cation patterns and the political behavior of the family members. They found that both the pluralistic parents and children appear to be more politically informed and active, and more often use the media for information. The pluralistic children are also the most active in the politically-relevant school activities of publication, speech and debate, and student government. In contrast, protective parents and their children tend to be low in public affairs competence. Both are heavy viewers of entertainment shows on television, but tend not to view public affairs television or read "hard" news in the newspaper. The children have low grade point averages, spend relatively little time with homework, and are the group least likely to participate in school activities. McLeod, Chaffee, and Wackman also investigated the relationship be- tween family communication patterns and the way people react to more specific situations. They devised an imbalanced situation of a liked person having a negative opinion of something the respondent favors. The respondent was then read a list of ten alternative reSponses, for which his subjective evaluation of the likelihood of each was recorded. They found for parents raised in a pluralistic home, there is a strong likelihood of communication as a response coupled with an unwillingness to withdraw directly or indirectly from the situation. This type of parent- is particularly likely to tell the neighbor why he is wrong, but believes he can do this without becoming angry. They also found that parents who establish protective or consensual communication environments with their children are relatively unlikely to give communication as a response to imbalance. 1'4 They are more likely than other types of parents to withdraw by forgetting the situation and by seeking authority. Hypotheses According to the findings just reviewed, the predictor variables in this study should maintain a consistent relationship with one another. If the predictor variables of "parent support of child," "child support of parents," and "child's contribution to the family discussion" are all re- lated to one of the dependent variables, such as, "child's influence on parents," the total set of relationships can be illustrated in Figure 1. Child's Influence on Parents (-) (+) (+) (-) (+) Child's Child's Parents' Support of Contribution Support of Parents Child I (_a I I Figure 1. The interrelationship of the three predictor variables and one of the four criterion measures. 15 Since the previous findings also suggest that the four dependent variables in this study are all positively related to one another, the above set of relationships should apply equally well to the other three dependent variables. The following sets of hypotheses are categorized separately for each dependent variable under study. Child's_perceived influence on parents Costley indicates that, the fewer negative social-emotional (non- supportive) statements made to group members, the greater a member's personal attraction to the group. Cartwright and Zander state that attracted members are more accepting of others' opinions and more often change their minds to take the views of fellow members. If it can be assumed that parents who make few non-supportive statements to their children will be inclined to make more supportive statements, the findings suggest: H1: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. Both Gibb and Likert suggest that a democratic leadership style involves giving the group members ample opportunity to express their thoughts without being constrained by the leader pressing his own views, implying a high tolerance for disagreement. Douvan and Adelson report that demo- cratic families tend to tolerate disagreement, while also having a highly attracted members. Since attracted members are more accepting of others' opinions and more often change their minds to take the views of fellow members, the findings suggest: 16 H2: The less support that a child gives his parents' ideas, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. Douvan and Adelson found that children from democratic homes indicate a high level of participation in family decisions. Since, in a democratic family, support and participation are related to personal attraction, and attracted members more readily try to influence others, the findings suggest: H3: The more a child contributes to the family discussion, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. Child's self-confidence in decision-making: The findings used in support of the first three hypotheses would also seem to reflect upon the development of a child's self-confidence. Thomas and Burdick (195”), and Cohen (1956) found that persons of high selféesteem exerted more influence attempts than persons of low self- esteem. With the relationship between self-esteem and influence suggested, similar hYPOtheSGS seem tenable for the development of a child's self- confidence can be equated. H4: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgment. H5: The less support that a child gives his parents' ideas, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgment. H6: The more a child contributes to the family discussion, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgment. 17 Parents control over a child's decision-making. Since perceived influence and self-confidence have been suggested as factors in the make-up of a democratic system, similar hypotheses seem tenable for the concept of parental control. Parental control, or actually the lack of it, is defined as the parent trusting the child to make the most of his own decisions. Costley reports fewer negative social—emotional (non-supportive) statements made, the greater the satisfaction with group decisions, suggesting that parents with democratic values might indicate greater satisfaction with the child's decisions. Sargent indicates that democratic leaders tend to make a high proportion of positive social-emotional (supportive) state- ments. He also found that democratic leaders phrased most of their con- tributions in the form of questions, implying a greater satisfaction with others' decisions. If it can be assumed that parents who make few non- supportive statements to their children will be inclined to make more supportive statements, then: H7: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more the parents will let their child make his own decisions. As long as the relationship between perceived influence and parental control has been suggested, the findings used in support of the hypotheses concerning perceived influence would also seem to apply to the concept of parental control. Thus, the findings suggest: H8: The less support that a child gives his parents' ideas, the more the parents will let their child make his own decisions. 18 H9: The more a child contributes to the family discussion, the more the parents will let their child make his own decisions. Child's information-seeking, Democratic families emphasize, among other things, that a child can make up his own mind without fear that reaching a different conclusion from his parents will endanger social relations in the family. McLeod states that pluralistic families exhibit the same characteristics, which suggests that both types have some behaviors in common. McLeod, Chaffee, and Wackman found that children from pluralistic homes are well informed and use the media for information. They also point out that pluralistic families encourage a child to explore new ideas and controversial material. It would seem that the rational used with per- ceived influence, selfeconfidence, and parental control might also apply to a child's information-seeking. Thus, the findings suggest: H10: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more often the child will seek information from personal and mediated sources. Hll: The less support that a child gives his parents' ideas, the more often the child will seek information from personal and mediated sources. H12: The more a child contributes to the family discussion, the more often the child will seek information from personal and mediated sources. CHAPTER II RESEARCH DESIGN General Study Design Data pertinent to the present study were collected in 25 personal interviews with families in the Holt, Michigan, area. Thus, an experimental situation in which experimental variables could be manipulated was not set up. Consequently, a correlational approach is used to test the hypotheses, based on measuring instruments derived from the responses obtained in the interview schedules. SamplinggProcedure The respondents selected to test the present hypotheses were 25 families with the following characteristics: (1) each family has a boy between in and 16 years-of-age; (2) each family consists of the boy's natural parents; and (3) that the boy and his parents have been living together, as a family. For families with more than one boy within the specified age limits, the youngster was selected at random before the interview. This procedure had to be used for only three families in the study. The sample was limited to boys and their parents for control purposes, since it would require a much larger sample to make adequate comparisons between boys and girls on the major variables under study. 19 20 Listings of families based on the above criteria were obtained from school census records. A final list was compiled of approximately 100 families which met the qualifications established by the investigator. A personal letter was sent to each family, explaining the purpose of the study and asking for their cooperation. (See Appendix A). After two or three days, the letter was followed by a phone call, to set up an appointment-time for the interview. Of the 100 families, one out of every four indicated that they would take part in the study. Four call backs were made on the remaining 75 families, with approximately 20% of the families not being contacted. 0f the remaining 60 families who were reached, most refusals were due to a conflict with summer vacation schedules and a general inability to get three family members together for the interview. Interviews were conducted over a seven-week period in the months of July and August, 1968. A description of the sample is given in Table 1. 21 Table 1. Description of Sample on Several Social and Demographic Attributes. Personal Characteristics Mother Father Age: 35 years or under 20% u% 36 - #0 years 32 #8 ul - #5 years 16 20 #6 - 50 years 29 20 51 years or over 8 8 100% 100% Median age for both mother and father - no years Education: Post- graduate work 0% 8% College graduate 8 u l - 3 years college 8 16 High school graduate 6H 56 9 - 11 years completed 20 8 8 years completed 0 u 1 - 7 years completed 0 u 100% 100% Median education for both mother and father - 12 years Total Annual Income for Family: $7,000 or less 9% $7,000 - $9,000 20 $9,001 - $11,000 16 $11,001 - $13,000 19 $13,001 - $15,000 25 $15,001 or over 16 100% Median income $12,500 Number of Children: 1 H% 2 29 3 16 u 20 5 16 6 l2 7 or more 8 100% Birth order: First 32% 1219916 as 22 Operationalization of Variables Interaction Measurement A modified version of Bales' interaction-process analysis system was used in this study. The twelve categories are shown in Figure 2. Perhaps the simplest way to conceive of an ideal problem-solving sequence is in terms of the four sections labeled A, B, C, and D. Section A contains several types of positive reactions, and Section D contains a similar group of negative reactions. Section B constitutes a group of attempted answers, while Section C constitutes a group of activities which can be characterized as questions. Using this conception, it can be suggested that the interaction process consists of questions, followed by attempted answers, followed by either negative or positive reactions. Unit of Analysis. The unit scored was the smallest discriminable segment of verbal or nonverbal behavior to which the coder could assign a classification under conditions of continuous serial scoring. This unit is referred to as an act. Often the unit was a single simple sentence expressing or conveying a complete thought. Complex sentences always involved more than one score. Dependent clauses were scored separately. Compound sentences joined by "and", "but", etc. were broken down into their component simple parts, each of which was given a score. The categories used are all-inclusive, in the sense that every act can be classified into a defined category. The method requires the coder to make a classification of every sequential act he observes. This is especially important since acts are interpreted according to the context in Social-Emotional Area: Positive Reactions Task Area: Attempted Answers Task Area: Questions Social-Emotional Area: Negative Reactions 23 1 Shows solidarity, raises other's status, gives help, reward Shows Tension Release jokes, laughs, shows satisfaction Agrees, shows passive acceptance, understands, concurs, complies Gives suggestions, direction, implying autonomy for other Gives inion, evaluation, analysis, expresses feeling, wish Gives orientation, information, repeats, clarifies, confirms Asks for orientation, information, repetition , confirmation Asks for gpinion, evaluation, analysIs, expression of feeling Asks for saggestion, direction, possiBIe ways of action 10 11 12 Dis recs, shows passive refection, formality, withholds help Shows tension, asks for help, withdraws out of field Shows ant onism, deflates other's status, defends or asserts self Figure 2. Bales' System of Observational Categories. 24 which they occur. Save for possible error, no acts are omitted from classification. The definitions of the interaction categories used by the coders in this study are provided in Appendix B. Coders. Two coders were trained to record the interaction in the family groups. The coders went through a series of training sessions conducted by the investigator in which they read and discussed the theoretical framework of the Bales' interaction process analysis system, read and discussed the definitions of the categories, and recorded the interaction on two pre-test tapes. For the final analysis, five tapes were selected at random from the 25 tapes used in the study. Agreement scores were computed between the two coders and between each coder and the investigator for these five tapes. Scores were obtained for the ibllowing three categories: Coder l Coder l Coder 2 with with with Coder 2 Investigator Investigator Support 78% 83% 8u% Contributions 86% 87% 84% Non-support 80% 79% 79% Since the agreement scores are fairly high, it seems reasonable to consider their use in constructing the indices of support. Indices of Support Bach act is scored in sequence according to Bales' method of inter- action process analysis. This score shows, first, who initiates the act and to whom it is directed. In addition, it indicates the relevance of the act either to the solution of the problem confronting the family or to the state of intergration of the family. Acts classified as relevant 25 primarily to the family problem (categories four through nine) are combined and called "contributions". Positive acts (categories one through three) directed specifically to other members in the family are called acts of "support"; negative acts (categories ten through twelve) directed specifically to others are called acts of "nonsupport." In this manner, interaction is divided into items offered to the family's solution and into positive or negative responses to what is offered. More precisely, the rates indicate the preponderance of supportive versus non-supportive acts, for the calculation of the rate of support takes into account both classes of acts. Support is assumed to range from positive (where supportive acts outnumber non-supportive) through zero to minus (where non-supportive acts outnumber supportive). Details in the calculation of rates of specific members in the family are given in the following formula. The rate at which member 1 supports member 2 (R312) is given by the equation: Agreement (support) Number of answers given Number of questions initiated Disagreement (non-support) R512 = 100 x A12 - D12 B2 + C2 UOW> Where: A12 refers to the frequency of supportive acts initiated by member 1 and directed to member 2. D12 refers to the frequency of non-supportive acts initiated by member 1 and directed to member 2. B2 and C2 combined refer to the frequency of "contributions" initiated by member 2 regardless of the recipients of the acts. For input into the above formula, the following measures of interaction were obtained: 1. Frequency of mother's support of child. 2. Frequency of father's support of child. 3. Frequency of child's support of mother. .u— 26 u. Frequency of child's support of father. 5. Frequency of mother's non-support of child. 6. Frequency of father's non-support of child. 7. Frequency of child's non-support of mother. 8. Frequency of child's non-support of father. 9. Frequency of mother's contributions. 10. Frequency of father's contributions. 11. Frequency of child's contributions. In addition to mother, father, and child support patterns, indices of support for primary and non-primary parent were calculated. Each child was asked to respond to the question: "On matters such as jobs and clothing, if you could ask for ideas from only one of your parents, which parent would you ask?" Thus, the child was given an opportunity to express a preferred interaction pattern. It was reasoned that the primary parent index might give a more reaistic picture of the actual parent-child interaction that occurs in the home. The primary parent index is composed of 15 mothers and 10 fathers. The average (mean) ratings and the variability of each support index are shown in Tables 2 and 3. Furthermore, the percentage of the child's contribution to the total family discussion was computed. The responses ranged from u% to 32%, with a mean response rating of 20%. Indices of Adolescent Autonomy The conceptual and Operational definitions along with some samples of the items used are: Child'sgperceived influence on parental decisions: The extent to which a child perceives himself as affecting decisions made by his mother and father. Operationally, it is the sum of scores a child obtains from his responses to questions dealing with whether his parents ask for his opinion on family decisions. "About how often would you say your parents ask you for your opinion on family decisions?" About once a week 27 For example, Less than once a month Once or twice a month Several times a week Table 2. Average (mean) rating and variability on parents' support of child. Parent Mother's Father's Primary Parent Non-primary Parent Support of Support Support Support of Support of Child Index of Child of Child Child Child +.u1 to +.50 -% -% -% -% +.31 to +.uo - - 4 - +.2l to +.30 - u u - +.ll to +.20 l2 l2 l2 8 .00 to +.10 nu N8 52 no -.01 to -.10 2H 2H 12 36 -.11 to -.20 20 8 12 16 -021 to -030 "' '4 ll - -.31 to -.HO - - - - -.ul to -.50 - - - - 100% 100% 100% 100% Average Rating .00 .03 .03 .00 A score of .00 is equal to "no support". 28 Table 3. Average (mean) rating and variability on child's support of parents' ideas. (N = 25) Child's Child's Child's Child's Child's Support of Support of Support of Support of Support of Parent Mother Father Primary Non-primary Index Parent Parent +.u1 to +.50 -% -% -% -% +.31 to +.'+0 - - - - +.2l to +.30 u - 1+ - +.ll to +.20 u 1+ 8 - .00 to +.10 an 56 56 HQ -.01 to -.10 118 32 32 #8 -.11 to -.20 - - - - -.21 to -.30 - 8 - 8 -.31 to -.H0 - - - - -.ul to -.50 .- - - - 100% 100% 3.60% "160% Average Rating .03 -.Ol .03 -.01 A score of .00 is equal to "no support". There are five items on which the child was asked to respond. For the "child's influence on mother" index, all ten of the inter-item correlations were in the right direction and ranged from .19 to .59, with a median correlation of .91. The inter-item correlations for the "child's influence on father" index were also in the right direction and ranged from .25 to .68, with a median corre- lation of .I+6. The average (mean) ratings and the variability of each in- fluence distribution are shown in Table It. Child's self-confidence in decision making: The extent to which a child feels that he has the ability and willingness to rely on his own strengths. Operationally, it is the sum of scores a child obtains from his responses to 28a questions dealing with how sure he is that he has made the best possible choice in a decision situation. For example, "If you had just joined a new club at school, how sure would you be that you had made the best choice possible?" Now sure Sure Very sure 29 Table 8. Average (mean) rating and variability on a child's perceived influence on his parents. (N = 25) Child's Child's Child's Child's Child's Influence Influence Influence Influence Influence on Parents on on on Primary on Non-Primary Index Mother Father Parent Parent 1? (high) 8% 8% 8% 8% 16 8 8 l2 - 15 8 8 l2 8 l8 - 8 - 8 l3 8 8 8 8 l2 8 - 8 - ll l2 l2 l2 12 10 20 8 16 12 9 - 8 - 8 8 12 12 8 20 7 8 - 8 - 6 8 8 8 8 5 - 8 - 8 u - - _ - 3 - - 8 - 2 8 8 8 - 1 - - - - 0(lmn 8 8 8 8 W9. E69. 1669. 3'69. Average Rating 9 . 7 (D O (I) H O O .f.‘ (D O N 30 "If your son needed new clothes for school... does he usually choose his own, or do you decide for him?" I always decide I often decide Every now and then I seldom decide I never decide Four items were used, and both mother and father responded separately to them. For the "father-child index of control," the six inter-item correlations ranged from -.02 to .65, with only one in the wrong direction. The median correlation was .08. The six inter-item correlations for the "mother-child index" ranged from -17 to .52, with only one in the wrong direction. The median correlation was .32. The average (mean) rating and the variability of each control distribution was shown in Table 5. Although the father-child control index has no homogeneity, findings pertinent to it will be reported. This problem will be discussed further in the last chapter. Child's information-seeking; The extent to which a child seeks ideas from both mediated and personal sources. Operationally, it is the sum of scores a child obtains from responses to questions dealing with how often he would look for ideas from both personal and mediated sources. e.g., "If you needed to buy clothes for school and uncertain as to what choice to make, how often would you look for... $3222 from newspapers? Often Now and then Seldom Never 31 Table 5. Average (mean) rating and variability on parents' control of child's decisions. (N = 25) Parent Mother's Father's Primary Non-primary Control of Control Control Parent Control Parent Control Child of of of of Index Child Child Child Child 18 (low control) 8% 8% 8% 8% 13 - 8 - 8 12 12 8 8 8 11 2O 8 8 2O 10 8 8 - 8 9 8 20 12 12 8 28 8 12 20 7 12 16 12 16 6 8 12 16 8 5 8 8 8 - 8 8 8 8 8 3 - 12 12 - 2 - - - - 1 - - - - 0 (high control) 100% 100% 150% 150% Average Rating 7.6 9.1 7.8 9.2 32 The information sources used can be classified as personal vs. mediated, on the basis of whether or not face-to-face contact is involved. Thus newspapers, television, magazines and movies fall into the mediated category and all direct contacts with mother, peers, father and relatives fall into the personal category. However, upon closer analysis, the data do not warrant differential predictions for these two types of sources. The six inter-item correlations for the four mediated sources ranged from -.30 to .62, with three negative cxarrelations. The median correlation was only .07. The three negative correlations were the relationship of newspapers to the other three mediated sources. Similarly, the six inter-item correlations for the four personal sources ranged from -.30 to .60, with three negative correlations. The median correlation was .18. This time, however, the three negative correlations were the relationship of peers to the other three personal sources. The data seem to suggest a somewhat different grouping of variables. For the mediated sources, it would seem to make sense to combine magazine television and movie usage into a single index. The three inter-item correlations are .36, .59, and .62. The average (mean) rating and variability of this index are shown in Table 6. Since newspapers were negatively correlated with the other three sources and accounted for a great deal of source usage, a separate index was constructed for news- papers. The average (mean) rating and variability of the newspaper index are shown in Table 7. 33 Table 6. Average (mean) rating and variability on selected mediated and personal source usage of child. (N = 25) Child's Child's Child's Combined Combined use Combined use Usage of Magazines, of Mother, Index TV and Movies. Father and Relatives 18 (high use) --% --% l7 -- -- l6 8 8 15 8 8 18 -- 8 13 -- l6 l2 8 -- 11 -- 8 10 8 8 9 l2 8 8 8 l6 7 12 8 6 12 8 5 l2 8 8 16 8 3 8 -- 2 -- -- l 8 -- 0 (low use) 8 8 100% Té'cT% Average Rating 6.8 to o O 38 Table 7. Average (mean) rating and variability on newspaper and peer usage of child. (N = 25) Child's Child's Child's Usage Use of Use of Index Newspaper Peers 6 (high use) 28% 88% 5 12 16 8 12 16 3 l2 l6 2 28 -- l 16 8 0 (low use) -- -- 100% 100% Average Rating 3.5 8.8 In regards to the personal sources, a separate index was constructed which included mother, father, and relatives. The inter-item correlations are .56, .57, and .60. The average (mean) rating and variability of this "family index" are shown in Table 6. Similarly, since peer usage was negatively correlated with the other personal sources, a separate "peer index" was constructed. this index are shown in Table 7. The average (mean) rating and variability of For an explanation of these usage 35 patterns, the work of Riley and Riley (1951) would suggest this difference, based upon whether the child is "family oriented" or "peer oriented." Data Collection Personal interviews were carried out in the home of each family. After some preliminary remarks to place everyone at ease, the interviewer introduced the family to the immediate task at hand, namely, their dis- cussion of various topics dealing with adolescent problems. The discussion topics selected were designed to meet several criteria. They had to be sufficiently interesting to the subjects to insure motivation for communication. They had to involve questions of social reality for which there were no "correct" answers. They had to permit several defensible solutions and to allow for differences of opinion among family members. They had to be topics on which all subjects would have adequate infermation to carry on a discussion. The interviewer read the following instructions: "Below are a series of situations which are discussed in most families at one time or another. Families seem to handle the problems in different ways. We would like for you to discuss among yourselves as many points of view that you are familiar with. From these different views, select a view which represents the thinking of your family. "Please spend some time with each of the situations. There is no right or wrong answer...only what your family feels is the most appropriate answer fer the situation. You will have 30 minutes to discuss all four situations below. Don't worry about the time; we will inform you when the half hour is up." The list of suggested discussion questions given to the subjects is reproduced in Appendix C. 36 After reading the instructions, the interviewer answered any questions that the subjects asked. At this point, the interviewer turned on the tape recorder and indicated that he would leave the room until they finished their discussion, or until the 30 minutes for discussion had elapsed. Actual discussion time ranged from 8 to 32 minutes, with an average (mean) time of 19 minutes. Upon completion of their discussion, the interviewer handed out a brief questionnaire to each member of the family. The complete questionnaires given the respondents are presented in Appendix D. After respondents had filled out the questionnaire, the interviewer again answered any questions that they had. This completed the respondent's participation in the study. CHAPTER III FINDINGS Description of Sample As previously stated, the original sample consisted of names and addresses of 100 families in the Holt, Michigan area. The interviewers were able to complete 25 interviews with families in the sample before field work halted late in August, 1968. No attempt was made to get a representative sample of Holt families. The objective of the study was not to obtain estimates of what the whole population of Holt families is like. Rather, it was to come up with a sample of families which had specific characteristics so that they could be compared on the behaviors under study. Half of the families reported an annual family income of at least $12,500. Families at this particular stage seem to be fairly well established; thus, the variability on the income figures did not seem to be wholly unrealistic. On age, mothers and fathers both averaged 80 years old. About 20 percent of the mothers were 35 or under, while only one of the fathers fell in this age bracket. In addition, only two of the mothers and fathers were over 50 years of age. This would be expected because parents of teenagers are likely to be under 50 years of age. 37 38 As for education, at least half of the mothers and fathers had finished high school. A sixth of the mothers had some college training, while a fourth of the fathers had been to college. On the other hand, none of the mothers, but two of the fathers never reached high school. Half of the mothers had at least four children. Bight percent reported families of seven or more children. The boys used in this study split fairly evenly in regards to birth order, with first and last born each representing about a third of the sample. By design, the boys in the sample ranged from 18 to 16 years of age, with the modal age being 15. Furthermore, they had all completed either their eighth or ninth year of school. Analytic Scheme As already mentioned, a correlational approach was used in testing the hypotheses. First, a simple index of relationship between each predictor variable and each criterion measure was obtained. This index indicates the zero-order correlation between two variables without potential con- ta minating factors being held constant. Before drawing conclusions from these findings, a check was made to determine whether the predictor variables were themselves interrelated. In regards to parental support patterns, the correlation between "mother's support of child" and "father's support of child" is relatively small (.15). In addition, "primary parent support of child" has only a slight correlation with "non-primary parent support of child" (.16). .‘. Furthermore, an analysis of the child's support patterns shows the correlation between the "childés support of mother" and the "child's support of father" to be moderate and in a negative direction (-.38). The same 39 relationship holds between the "child's support of primary parent" and the "child's support of non-primary parent" (-.38). In order to check on spurious relationships, partial correlation analysis was used to hold variables constant while the relationship between two other variables was studied. Thus, it was possible to partial out the influence attributable to "father's support of child" while checking on the relationship between "mother's support of child" and the "child's perceived influence on mother." Also, this approach was used to control out the influence attributable to the "child's support of non-primary parent." The stability of the relationship also had to be determined.* With a sample size of 25, a correlation coefficient of .80 would occur simply because of sampling error only five in one hundred times and a correlation coefficient of .51 would occur by chance only once in one hundred times. In addition, the zero-order correlations were checked to determine whether the assumption of linearity was reasonable.. Tests of Hypotheses The findings are categorized according to the four criterion measures under study. Thus, there are four sets of findings with each set containing three hypotheses. Child's perceived influence on_parents. Parents' support of child. The first hypothesis was concerned with *The formula of the test can be found in McNemar (1952, p. 167). The null hypothesis tested was that the true, i.e., the population r equals zero. 80 the parents' support of child during a conversation and the child's per- ceived influence on them: H1: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. Data pertinent to this hypothesis are presented in Table 8. It would seem that, the more support the child receives from his 223233. or designated primary parent, the greater his perceived influence on his parents (.56, .80, .66, and .58). Before drawing such a conclusion, partial correlation coefficients were computed for each predictor variable. Support by his father or his non-primary parent did not significantly affect his judgment of influencing his parents. The partial correlation indices are almost the same as the zero-order correlations for mother and primary parent. (.58, .38, .65, and .58). As Table 8 shows, three of the partial correlation coefficients were greater than would be expected from sampling error. The fourth was close to significant. In other words, the findings were in the predicted direction with three correlation coefficients reaching the .01 significance level. Thus, the hypotheses was supported for mother and primary parent support, but was not for father and non-primary parent support. Child's support of parents. The second hypothesis was concerned with the child's support of parents and.his perceived influence on them: H2: The less support that the child gives his parents' ideas, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. Iain-n-—.l I - re,-o..- - 0......) his. en- .—eL-e-,- M shy. lit n. -\ ‘ Vxnfiufi uses-\f\§ .L . TN-‘.~ [.0 !. a§I\ ~, Ho.VQ «a. mo.v.a « cameo mo anomosn panama hamsfinm so.t so. sawed mo uncoonn panama sensauotcoz pawgo mo pnooosm panama hamemotcoz «awn. eemm. oawno mo unooosm panama hnmsanm ”can panama hnmenmtco: co endurance n.oaw£o consume ofinmcofiumaom cameo mo Macadam panama hhMEwnm no.1 mo. pawno mo phoneme panama runawnotcoz oawno mo uncooam panama mumeHnotcoz sumo. «amp. oawno mo phonon» panama seesanm "one posh—em Nuns—rum co endurance n.pafino consume ofizmcofipmaem .o pawns mo Phaedra m.nonuoz am. em. pawno mo whooosn n.90numm cameo mo “scoops n.no£uem mm. «as. pawns mo uncoosn c.902uoz 81 ”one ponumm co oocesamcfi m.eawno consume manmcowpeaom eases mo pnoaaam e.aoeuoz as. ea. cases no enemas» m.noeuaa oaweo mo phonon» n.90nymm seam. «eon. oaweo mo phoneme n.no£poz ”one nonuoe co oocuaamcm m.odmno escapee owsmooHumaom mammmec< cowumaonnoo cowweaonnoot cowpmaonnoo Hewpnom cm uempmcoo Hmwpnem nonmetonou odor enouuum .muconmo co.oo:osamc« oo>fioonoo n.oaweo one peace we woodman .mpconoo consume nanncoapmaontta nanonwoosm mo woos .m magma 82 H O .V 9.3.» mo.u.c e panama macawno mo whooorm w.oaw£o seem. same. panama kanHnatao: mo whooosm m.oH«£o HN.I Roz... Magma sausage mo uncoosm m.oawzo panama humesotcoo mo upooosm m.oawno "can panama hamsmpotco: so mucosamcw m.pawno consume ownmcowpmaom panama hhmeno mo phooasm m.oawno seam. seam. panama hemefinotcoc mo upcooom m.pamno m.odwno em:.t eemm.t panama message mo phoneme m.oawno panama macawpotcoc mo whooosn ”new unused mum5wpo co ooaosamcfi m.oaw:o consume ownncowpmaom nogvmm mo uncoosn n.oawzo servos mo pecans» m.eaaeo mm. «as. Hm.t nuance mo anemone m.oawno nosumm mo «nooosm u.oHHno oa.t ”one nonumm co cocosamcm m.oawno escapee ownmcofiumaom sonuom mo uncoosm m.eaa£o fiQSHOE m0 Pfioagflm m.UHH£U flamm. flflom. nonumm mo whooosm n.oawno mm.t mm.t nonuos mo uncoorn m.eawno ”new nuance co cocosamca m.oawso consume ownncofipmaom mwmswmc< cowumaonnou coaumaonnoo cowumaonwoo Hmwvnmm nonfictonoN Huapnma as nempmeoo can: enouomm .nuconeo was no codename“ oesfioonoo n.oa«co a cam m.eafigo m sweeper afiemeofipwaonsum mamwnuoasz mo paws .m wanes museums was mo woodman 83 Six out of the eight zero-order correlations were stable estimates (Table 9). Out of the six correlations, only two were in the predicted direction, while the other four are in the wrong direction to support the hypothesis. As Table 9 shows, the partial correlation indices are somewhat different from the zero-order correlations. The data indicate that, the 'more the child supports his father or designated non-primary parent, the greater his perceived influence on his parents (.53, .39, .51, and .60). It would also seem that, the l£§§_a child supports his primary parent, the greater his perceived influence on the primary parent (-.83). Four of the eight partial correlation coefficients reached the prior-set significance level. However, the hypothesis was not confirmed, since three of the four significant correlations were in the wrong direction. Child's contribution. The third hypothesis was concerned with the percentage of the child's contribution to the family discussion and his perceived influence on his parents: H3: The more the child contributes to the family discussion, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. The four zero-order correlations were not greater than would be expected from sampling error (Table 10). All the relationships were also in the wrong direction. Thus, the hypothesis was not confirmed. Child's selfeconfidence in decision-making. Parents' support of child. The fourth hypothesis was concerned with a parents' support of child and the child's self-confidence in his own judgments : 88 Table 10. Test of Hypothesis 3--relationship between the percentage of a child's contribution and a child's perceived influence on his parents. Zero-order Correlation Relationship between the percentage of child's contribution and: Child's influence on mother -.32 Child's influence on father -.18 Child's influence on primary parent -.l8 Child's influence on non-primary parent -.29 H8: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgments. The zero-order correlations between the variables are presented in Table 11. Three out of the four relationships are stable estimates. The data indicate that mother's support of child, father's support of child, and primary parent support of child have moderate bearing on the likeli- hood that the child will have self-confidence (.68, .80, and .65). A comparison between the zero-order correlation and the partial correlation indicates only a negligable reduction in the simple correlations (.68, .80, and .68). Again, with a sample size of 25, the three correlation coefficients did not fall within the expected range of sampling error. Thus, the hypothesis was supported for mother, father, and primary parent support. 85 Ho.Vm «a mo.uva « cameo mo whoaosm panama humefipm bu. om. paago mo whooosm panama sensanotcoz cameo mo whoooam panama aNMEwnotaoz seem. eemm. pawno mo whooosn panama hamswnm adage mo scones. u.nmepoz «as. «as. adage mo unease. m.amepma 320 mo Eocene 9858 «.30. 33¢. 3% mo Eocene $86.02 ”new cocooameootmaon n.oaweo consume ownncOMHMHom mwnwamcc coHumaonemm-t --cmmwoaoenoo cofipmaohnoo Hmwvnmm cm ecmumcoo Hmaunmm noonouonou can: nnouomm .ooeoeameooumaoo m.eaaeo a sea adage so whooosn .nuccneo escapee manncowueaontt: manoeuomhm mo pros .HH earns 86 Child's support of parents. The fifth hypothesis was concerned with a child's support of his parents and the child's self-confidence in his own judgments: H5: The less support the child gives his parents' ideas, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgment. Again, three out of four zero-order correlations were stable estimates (Table 12). Two out of three of the correlations were, also, in the pre- dicted direction. However, when partial correlation coefficients were computed, only one correlation coefficient reached the prior-set significance level. As in the previous cases, the primary parent concept remains a fairly consistent predictor of the child's behavior. It would seem that the less a child supports his primary parent, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgments (-.51). The data suggest support of the hypothesis only when phrased in terms of the primary parent. Child's contribution. The sixth hypothesis dealt with the percentage of the child's contribution to the family discussion and the child's self- confidence in his own judgments: H6: The more the child contributes to the family discussion, the more the child will have confidence in his own judgments. A simple index of relationship between the percentage of the child's contribution to the family discussion and the child's selfeconfidence was obtained. The data indicate that there was virtually no relationship between these two indices (-.02). Thus, the hypothesis was not confirmed. 87 Ho .V new mo.v as panama hamswno mo encoder n.oafino on. «we. panama samefinotcoc.mo “sconce m.oawno panama humewnotcoc mo Raccoon n.oawno seam.t eeom.t panama znmEHao mo phonon» m.pH«no 852. .8 Home... 9320 as. mm. 88.8 no €883 .836 nozumm mo phonon» n.pawno om.t «33.: nonuoe mo unooosn m.oawno ”one cocooamcootmaon n.oafino coozpontownchMumaom mwmsmwc< newumaonnoo .1 cowumaonnoo cowumaonnoo Hmwupmm cw pompmcoo ammunmm aspectonon can: muouomm .oooeofimcooamaom m.eaaco m can unsound can no woodman n.oawno o coospon ownmcoaumaonttm nwmcnpoobm mo Home .NH canoe 88 Parent control over a child's decision-making, Parents' support of child. The seventh hypothesis was concerned with the parents' support of child and the degree of control parents exert over their child's decisions: H7: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more the parents will let their child make his own decisions. Data pertinent to this hypothesis are presented in Table 13. Only two of the eight zero-order correlations were stable estimates. When the partial correlations were analyzed, both relationships remained greater than would be expected from sampling error. The findings suggest the more support the father gives his child, the more the mother will let the child have a say in his own decision-making (.83). Furthermore, the data indicate that primary parent support of child has a fairly strong bearing on the likelihood that the primary parent will let the child have a say in his own decision-making (.71). Thus, the hypothesis received partial support. Childs support of parents. The eighth hypothesis was concerned with the child's support of his parents and the degree of control parents exert over their child's decisions: H8: The less support the child gives his parents' ideas, the more the parents will let their child make his own decisions. The zero-order correlations between the variables are presented in Table 18. Only one out of the eight relationships was greater than would be expected from sampling error. A comparison between the zero-order correlations 89 Hoo.nvaee mo.V aw oawno mo uncoorm panama hhmEanm Ha. mo. pawco mo unooonu panama znmeaaotaoz pawno mo “wooden «comma hnmsanotooz Hm.t om.t pawns mo whooosm panama humeapm ”new neoHnfiooo n.paweo po>o achucoo ucoamolwmesfinmtcoc consume ownchwumaom cameo mo uncoosm penned sneewpm ma. mm. oawno mo uncoosm pounce sprawnotcoz sawed mo phonon» panama zhmefiaotcoz «ear. «ems. pagan mo whooosm panama mneawnm ”one meow» one 9323 no>o Hosanna pecan San cassava owemcoaumaom refine mo phonon» n.90cpoz «me. «3:. peace we uncoosm m.eeeumm pawno mo phonon» n.90numm mo. Ha. pawno mo uncoorn n.nonvoz ”one macamwooo n_mam:o no>o Hosanna n.9ozuoe coospon ownncowpmaom oafino mo phonon» n.noeuoz mo. oa. pawns mo uncoosn n.noepmm pawno mo phonon» m.nonumm mm. em. pawso mo woodman m.aonuoz ”one acoamwooo n_mamwo no>o Honvcoo n.posumm consume owechMHmHom ownsamcc cowueaonaoo cofivmacnaoo coaumaonnoo Howupmm cw panamcoo Hewunem neoAOtouoN ea»: maouoeu mo uncoosm m.uconmo presume oasecowuoaontns mwmccuoozm mo pace .eafizo no>o Honucoo .muccsmo one oafigo .ma manna 50 monUvmw panama massage mo uncoorm n.oawno mo. mo. panama unrefinotaoc mo unooosm n.oaw:o panama mNMEwaotco: mo pnooosn n.pa«£o so. no. panned hpmswno mo whooosm m.oawno ”one moowmwooo m.oawno ho>o Honucoo panama unusahotco: consume ownncowumacm panama unusaao mo whooorm m.pdfino on. sun. panama sueEMAQIcoc mo uncoorn n.odwzo unease macawnotcoc mo phoneme m.pafico mm.t e::.t panama sausage mo phonodn n.oawno ”one ecownwoeo n.oawgo no>o Houpcoo panama unnamed censure ownmcoHumaom 859. no Home; 3320 8.- 2.- possum mo Home... 9320 assume no Home; 9320 3. S. 8:6... .8 p.883 3320 "can uncanwooo p.320 nmso Honucoo m.no:uoe consume magmcowpmaom servos mo pecans». 9320 mm. «as. 83.3 we Home.» 9320 eunuch mo phonosn n.oaweo 50.: Hm.t nonvoe_mo pnomosm u.pawzo ”one maowmwooo m.oawno no>o Hoppcoo n.aonpmm consume ofismcowumaom ownsmec< cowpmaesaoo ----.,mmwvmaonuoo cowvmaonnoo Hmwuamm cw unmanaoo Hmaunom noonononow odor unapomm was mo phonon» e.oaano e consume .eawgo no>o donpcoo .muconeo oasecowueaoeutm mfimonuoomz mo pace .za magma 51 and the partial correlations suggests that the child support patterns have only a slight influence on the degree of control parents exert over decisions affecting a child. Thus, the hypothesis was not confirmed. Child's contribution. The ninth hypothesis was concerned with the percentage of the child's contribution to the family discussion and the degree of control parents exert over their child's decisions: H9: The more the child contributes to the family discussion, the more the parents will let their child make his own decisions. The zero-order correlations were not greater than would be expected from sampling error (Table 15). The data indicate virtually no relationship between the variables. Thus, the hypothesis was not confirmed. Table 15. Test of Hypothesis 9--relationship between the percentage of a child's contributions and parents' control over child. Zero-order Correlation Relationship between the percentage of child's contributions and: Mother's control over child .01 Father's control over child -.01 Primary parent control over child -.11 Non-primary parent control over child .15 52 Child's information-seekipg, Parents' support of child. The tenth hypothesis was concerned with a parents' support of child and the child's use of personal and mediated sources for ideas: H10: The more support parents give their child's ideas, the more often the child will seek information from personal and mediated ’sources. The zero-order correlations for the personal source variables are presented in Table 17. Two out of the eight correlations were stable estimates. However, when the partial correlations were computed, only one correlation coefficient reached the prior—set significance level. The data suggest that the greater the primary parent support of the child, the more the child will use his mother, father, and other relatives in obtaining information (.60). Similarly, the zero-order correlations for the mediated sources are presented in Table 16. Again, two out of eight zero-order correlations were stable estimates. A comparison between the zero-order correlations indicates only a slight reduction in the simple correlations. The data suggest that father's support of child and primary parent support of child have a moderate bearing on the likelihood that a child will use television, movies and magazines as sources of information (.50 and .57). Thus, the hypothesis received partial support. 53 HO.V.A¢¢ mo.V.A¢ pawso mo uncoosn panama macawnm 00. ca. pawno mo phonon» panama unmefinotgoz cameo mo uncoosm panama sausanotcoz seem. eemm. sawed mo «wooden unease sausage pawno mo uncoosm m.nonpoz «om. seam. sawed mo “sconce m.nonumm cameo mo whooosm n.9ozumm ma. ma. pawso mo phonon» m.nonuoz ”can nocwnmmoa one .mo«>oa .>e mo and cesarean u.oawno m censure awcmcoHumHom pawco mo whooosn panama hAMEahm mH.t Ha.t pawno mo uncoosn panama mumefinotcoz pawno mo uncoozm uconmo hnmsapotcoz ea. ma. pawno mo phonon» panama seesaam oafico mo whooonm n.nonuoz :H.t oa.t adage mo phonon» n.aogumm cameo mo «hoods» n.9onumm hm. mm. oaano mo «accuse n.90npoz ”new randomness mo on: 93.on m comrade owemcowueaem manmwmcc coaumaehhoo .. coupeaonnoo cofipnaonhoo Hewunmm a“ ucmuncoo defiance neonatonoN can: naouoom .uoonrou oouemoos no on: m.paano n can padre mo woodman .npeoneo consume ownmoofiunaosttoa nfiuonuoohz mo Home .oa manna su pauco mo ucooocn uconmo mnmauum pause mo ucooocm ucocmc scrawcotcoz peace mo ucooocm m.cocuoz pause mo ucoooc» m.cocumm pause mo uncoocn ucecmo zumeucm cause mo uncoocm ucocmm macawcotcoz sauce mo uncoocm u.cocuoz pause mo uncoocn n.cocuew Ho.Vcea move. «0.: mo. sauce mo uncoocn ucocmo succucotcoz aeoo. «com. sauce mo uncomcn uconeo sarcasm mm. «as. pause mo ucooocw m.cocucm am. so. cause mo uncoocn m.cocuoz coco no>wumaoc ecu .cocumm .cocuoa no can n.oawco m coosuoc oucncofiumaom ma. on. cause mo uncoocm ucocmo humeucotcoz mo. NH. pawco mo ucooocn uconmo humEucm mm. :m. pause mo unococn m.cocuom mH.t Ha.t cause mo uncomcn m.cocuoz “ecu 0mm cso mac mcocuo mo on: m.pawco e consume oucmcowumaom nwmudmc< cowumaoccoo Homucmm cw ucmumcoo can: acouomh MO acuumwvccou ecuueaoccoo Heuucem concatocou .nooccom Hmconcoo mo and m.oauco o ecu sauce uncoocn .mucoceo consume oucmcouucaocttoa manocuoosm mo umoa .ba manna 55 Child's support of parents. The eleventh hypothesis dealt with the child's support of his parents and his use of personal and mediated sources for ideas: H11: The less support the child gives his parents' ideas, the more often the child will seek information from personal and mediated sources. In regards to the personal-source variables, two out of eight zero- order correlations were stable estimates, but not in the right direction to support the hypothesis (Table 18). The partial correlations are only slightly smaller than the zero-order correlations. The data indicate that the greater the child's support of father and his support of the non- primary parent, the more the child will use his mother, father, and other relatives as sources of information (.81 and .51). The zero-order correlations concerning the mediated sources were not greater than would be expected from sampling error (Table 19). The data suggest only a slight relationship between the variables. The hypothesis was not confirmed, since the only two significant relation- ships were in the wrong direction. Child's contribution. The twelfth hypothesis dealt with the per- centage of the child's contribution to the family discussion and his use of personal and mediated sources for ideas: H12: The more the child contributes to the family discussion, the more often the child will seek information from personal and mediated sources. The four zero-order correlations were not greater than would be expected from sampling error (Table 20). Three out of four of the relation- ships were in the predicted direction, however. The hypothesis was not confirmed. lulaI-ll-I-I- e..- I-l-.eu: U El~ Pi N -. Ian I!!- OI...- U‘ u ltltvus cave-\fus b It'ue.~ Uli .NI. \ Hogv 92s mo.V ca ucocmo znmauco mo uncoocm o.oauco seam. eemm. ucocoo auosacotcoc mo uncoocm m.oauco ucmcoa mascucctcoc mo uncoocn o.oawco mm.t mm.t ucocoo sausage mo uncoocm m.oauco 859.. .3 Home: ”.320 «3. «S. 85.3 so Home... ”.320 cocumm mo ucooocm m.oawco mo.t :m.t cocuos_mo uncoocm o.paaco coco mo>uumamc oco .cocumu .cmcuoe no on: o.oa«co m cmosuoc oucmcowumaom ucocmo "w succeed mo ucooocm m.oawco mo.t mo.u ucoceo scmeucotcoc mo uncooom n.oaaco ucocoo Resonances mo 3.83:» 9320 so. 2. page >853 mo Haas; Page 86.8 .8 Home; «.320 3... 3... assume no Home; «.320 cocuom mo uncooco m.oauco ma. wm. cmcuoc mo ucooocm o.odmco coco owe czo mac ococuo we on: m.oauco o cooxuoc oucncowumamm onNHmc< cowumaoccoo ocuumamchoo coHumaoccoo Heuuhmm cu ucmuocoo Hmuucmm coonouocou odor ecouomm .omoccom Heconcoo no on: m.oauco o ocm mucocmo mo uncoocm m.oauco comsuoc oncchuuoHocttHH oumocuooum mo uoou .ma oacmu .01-11.naa’6 1.800.050 D II.- .h’t-n. 'O\~ t 5 ...C.§ un.-.§\A.- crew 57 uconmo ucmcmo ucmcmo unscacotcoc mo uncoocn m.pawco ucmcoo suoeuco mo uncoocm o.cawco canyon mo unease» u.eaceo 85% so «some.» .636 ”com nocwnowoe ocm .oow>oe .>u no on: n.oHHco m cmosuoc oucmcowuoaom ucocmo hcoeucatcoc mo uncoocm m.oawco ucmnmo anosaco mo usages» m.oauco cocuom mo uncoocm o.oawco cocuoe mo uncoocm m.pa«co coco commomsoc no one o.oauco o comsuoc oucocOwumHom sausage no unease. m.scaeo so. ma. ucocmo hnoEHco neon mo unease. m.ecaeo mm.- sm.- mosses no phones. a.eaceo ~o.t so. assume mo aromas. m.eaceo am.u mm.u mum5w9o_mo ucooocm m.eawco mm. mm. ucmcoo sarcasm neon mo ucoaasm m.eaaeo mm. ma. segues mo unease» o.ecaeu cm. «a. assume no unease» m.ecaeo mm. as. mmmwwoc< coaumamccoo .Lcowumaoccoo cowuoaoccoo t Hosanna ea ueepmeoo finances cacao-ones odor mcouoom .mooccom oouowome.mo on: m.oafico m vcm mucocmo mo uncooco m.oaaco coosumc oucmcoauoaocttaa mwmocuoo>r mo umme .ma manna 58 Table 20. Test of Hypothesis l2--relationship between the percentage of a child's contribution and a child's use of both mediated and personal sources. Zero-order Correlation Relationship between the percentage of a child's contribution and: Child's use of newspaper -.ou Child's use of TV, radio, and magazines -.16 Child's use of others his own age .21 Child's use of mother, father, and relatives -.22 CHAPTER IV CONCLUSIONS Sumary ,; One of the primary functions of the family is the preparation of a child to leave home. This study focused on the question as to whether or not there are characteristics of the communication between family members that are associated with the child's growth toward emancipation from - the family. In order to determine the characteristics of the communication behavior that occurs between family members, personal interviews were conducted in the home of each family. The child's interaction with both his mother and father was analyzed. In addition each child was asked to select the one parent from whom he generally preferred to seek advice. The chosen parent's reaponses were then analyzed in a "primary parent" index. It was felt that the interaction between the child and his primary parent would be a good predictor of the child's development, since this parent would have more of an opportunity to influence the child's behavior. A tape recording was made of the family members discussing various topics that dealt with adolescent problems. Bach discussion was analyzed using a modified form of Bales' interaction process-analysis categories. The interaction indices showed, first, who initiated the communication and, second, to whom it was directed. In this manner, interaction was divided into statements 59 60 offered to the family's solution of the problem and into positive or negative responses to what was offered by each member. After completion of their dis- cussion, each family member was asked to make judgments concerning the child's level of autonomy. Douvan et. al. states that one of the key concepts in the growth and development of the child is the notion of autonomy. Adolescent autonomy focuses on behavior and decisions. How much perceived influence does the 4 child have on his parents' decisions? 'How selfeconfident is he in his own i decision-making ability? What decisions can he make for and by himself? How often and from what sources does the child seek advice? The child may show his growth in a number of ways but these four criterion measures were investigated in this study. The respondents selected for this study were 25 families which have the following characteristics: (1) each family has a boy between 1n and 15 years-of-age; (2) each family is composed of the boy's natural parents; and (3) the boy and his parents are living together in the same household. The findings were grouped according to the four criterion measures under study. In order to check Spurious relationships, partial correlation analysis was used to partial out the influence attributable to one variable while the relationship between two. other variables was studied. For example, it was possible to partial out the influence attributable to "father's support of child" in a discussion while checking on the relationship between "mother's support of child" in a discussion and the "child's perceived in- fluence on mother." 61 Child's perceived influence on parents Parents' support of child. The first hypothesis concerned the relation- ship between the parents' reinforcement of their child's ideas when discussing family problems and the child's judgment as to his influence on his parents in general. The analysis showed the more support the child received from his umnher’in a discussion, the greater the child's perceived influence on her. Alfihough not statistically significant, a similar trend was found between the child and his father. Furthermore, the primagy parent's support of the child's ideas (i.e., the parent from which the child prefers to seek advice) explained more of the child's perceived influence on his parents than did the mother's or father's support, when they were studied separately. The hypothesis was supported as it pertains to mothers and primary parents. Child's support of parents. The second hypothesis stated that, the less the child supports his parents' ideas in a discussion, the more the child will perceive that he influences his.parents. The findings suggested the less the child supports his primagy parent in a discussion, the greater his perceived influence on this parent. However, the data also indicated that the l£§§_the child disagrees with his father, the more he will perceive himself as influencing both his mother and his father. There seems to be a trend for the child to find it easier to show disagreement with his mother's ideas, while showing moderate support of his father's ideas in a discussion. Therefore, the evidence only supports the hypothesis as it pertains to primary parents. Child's contribution. Also hypothesizid has the notion that, the more the child contributes to a family discussion, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents. The hypothesis was not confirmed by the data. 62 Child's self-confidence in decision-making Parents' support of child. The first hypothesis concerned the relation- ship between the parents' support of their child's ideas in a discussion and the child's indication of his perceived self-confidence in his own judgments. The analysis suggested the more the mother, father, or primary parent support the child in a discussion, the more the child will indicate that he is self- confident in his own decisions. The hypothesis was supported as it pertains to mother , father, and primary parents. Child's support of Jarents. The second hypothesis stated that, the less the child supports his parents in a conversation, the more the child will indicate that he has self-confidence in his own judgments. The findings indicated the less the child supports his primagz parent in a discussion, the more the child perceives that he has self-confidence. A similar negative relationship was found in regards to the child's support of his mother in a discussion; while the relationship was slightly positive in regard to the child's support of his father in a discussion. The data suggest support of the hypothesis as it pertains to mothers and primary parents. ghéild'sgcontribution. The final hypothesis of the series. suggested themore the child contributes to the family discussion, the more the child will indicate that he has self-confidence. It was not confirmed by the data. Parent control over a child's decision-making Parents' support of child. It was hypothesized that, the more support parents give their child's ideas in a discussion, the more the parents claim that they let their child make his own decisions. The data indicated that the primafl parent's support of the child in a conversation has a fairly 63 strong bearing on the likelihood that the primary parent perceives that he lets the child have a say in his own decisions. Also the data suggested that the more a mother claims that she lets her child make his own decisions, the more the father tends to support the child's ideas in a discussion. Similarly, the more a father perceives that he lets his child make his own decisions, the more the mother supports the child's ideas in a discussion. Only in the case r of the primary parent was there a strong positive relationship between the control exerted by a particular parent and the amount of support given to the child by that same type of parent. The hypothesis was supported as it pertains to primary parents. Child's support of parents. The less support the child gives his parents' ideas in a discussion, the more the parents perceive that they let their child make his own decisions. There was a slight tendency toward this hypothesized relationship between the primary parent and the child. However, since the correlation coefficient did not reach the pre-defined significance level, the; hypothesis was not confirmed. Child's contribution. The third hypothesis in this series stated the more the child contributes to the family discussion, the more the parents perceive that they let their child make his own decisions. It was not supported. Child's information-seekipg' Parents' support of child. The hypothesis stated that, the more support parents give their child's ideas in a discussion, the more often the child claims that he seeks information from personal and mediated sources. News- papers, television, magazines and movies fall into the mediated category and all direct contacts with mother, father, others his own age, and relatives fall 61+ ‘. into the personal category. The analysis suggested that, the greater the primary parent support of the child in a discussion, the more the child per- ceives that he uses his mother, father, and other relatives for information. In addition, the data indicated father and primary parent support of the child in a discussion have a moderate bearing on the likelihood the child uses television movies, and magazines as sources. On the other hand, the child's use of news- papers and peers for ideas showed only a very slight relationship with parental uni—- support of the child's ideas in a conversation. The hypothesis was supported as it pertains to primary parents. 3 ‘V'l‘. Child's support of parents. It was next hypothesized that, the £3 support the child gives his parents' ideas. in a discussion, the more often the child claims that he seeks information from personal and mediated sources. The findings suggested the greater the child's. support of his father's ideas in a discussion, the more the child indicated his general use of mother, father, and the other relatives as sources of information. However, the relationship was not in the hypothesized direction. Furthermore, there were no significant relationships concerning the mediated sources. Actually, the child's use of newspapers showed a slight positive relationship with an increase in the child's support of his parents' ideas in a discussion; while an increase in the child's ' use of television, movies, and magazines varied inversely with support of his parents' ideas in a discussion. Thus, the hypothesis was not confirmed. Child's contribution. The final hypothesis stated the more the child contributes to the family discussion, the more often the child claims that he seeks information from personal and mediated sources. The hypothesis was not confirmed. 6S Interpretation of Findings The direction of adolescent growth is clearly toward emancipation from the family. The period begins with the child almost entirely dependent on the family, needing its say-so for what he can and cannot do, still clinging to their ideas and ideals. It ends with the child reaching into adulthood, freer to make up his mind about what he will and will not do, holding his own beliefs andgvalues. ”The particular freedoms the child is given are the visible part of a larger conversation between parent and child, having to do with the child's need for later independence. It was the purpose of this study to analyze the relationships between parent-child communication in family conversations and the degree to which the child claims to have achieved inde- pendence in his thoughts and actions. A number of findings supported the hypotheses growing out of the ex- ploratory framework. Some did not. Of the twelve hypotheses tested, five were partially supported (H: l, u, 5, 7, 10). In regard to these hypotheses, it turned out that the best predictor of the child's~perceived indpendence was his communication style with his primagy parent. 'Of the remaining seven hypotheses, two were not in the predicted direction (H: 2 and 11), and the other five were not supported at all (H: 3, 6, 8, 9, 12). Most of these hypotheses were concerned with the percentage of the child's contribution to the family discussion, which seems to be a very poor predictor of the child's perceived independence. In this situation, the researcher can ask whether it was the theory or his Operationalization of the theory that led to non-significant results. 66 Conceptual Evaluations A basic consideration here is the assumed interrelationship of the concepts in this study. Lack of earlier empirical evidence utilizing these concepts in similar circumstances forced the hypotheses to be dram in such a manner that the relationships assumed had only indirect support from existing ...v~I literature. However, data from this study should provide some insight into the assumptions underlying the conceptual framework used. Are all of these A... indices of a child's increased feeling of independence from his parents related to a fair degree? [FT , Interrelationship of the criterion measures. Implicit in the formulation of the hypotheses was the assumed positive relationship between the child's perceived influence on his parents, the child's perceived self-confidence, parents' perceived control over the child, and the child's claimed information- seeking behavior. The relationship between the child's perceived influence and his self; confidence can be shown as follows: Zero-order Child's self-confidence and: Correlation Child's perceived influence on mother .69 Child's perceived influence on father .68 Child's perceived influence on primary parent .78 The finding of Thomas and Burdick (1951;), and Cohen (1956) that persons of high self-esteem attempt to exert more influence has received additional support. The assumption that the concepts of self-esteem and self-confidence have some overlapping behavioral characteristics seems reasonable. 67 The relationship between the child's perceived influence on his parents and the control parents indicate they exert over their child is somewhat tenuous at this time, due to the weak measures of parental control. However, fer the index which tapped the extent of control exerted by the primary parent over the child, the six inter-item correlations ranged from .0“ to .65, with the median correlation being .36. The relationships are as follows: The degree to which the primary parent lets the child Zero-order make his own decisions and: Correlation Child's perceived influence on mother .su Child's perceived influence on father .75 Child's perceived influence on primary parent .80 The data reflect this in the finding that primary parent support of the child's ideas in a discussion has a fairly strong bearing on the likelihood that the primary parent will let the childhave a say in decisions that concern him. Also, as would be expected, the more the primary parent indicates that he would let the child make his own decisions, the greater the child's judgment as to hiw own selfeconfidence in decision-making (.76). Finally, the relationship between the child's perceived influence on his parents and the child's use of various sources for ideas is presented. The data indicate only a slight relationship between the child's claimed use of either newspapers or peers for ideas and his perceived influence on his parents. In contrast, however, the child's use of mother, father, and relatives or his use of television, magazines and movies for ideas seems to show a moderate relationship to the child's perceived influence on his parents. 68 Child's me of TV, movies and magazines Zero-order for ideas and: Correlation Child's perceived influence on mother .3“ Child's perceived influence on father .33 Child's perceived influence on primary parent .142 Actually, only the relationship between the child's claimed use of television, movies, and magazines for ideas and the child's perceived influence on his primary parent is greater than would be. eXpected from sampling error. A much stronger relationship is indicated between the child's claimed use of mother, father, and relatives for ideas and the extent to which the child feels he influences his parents . Child's use of mother, father, and relatives Zero-order for ideas and: Correlation Child's perceived influence on mother .65 Child's perceived influence on father .77 Child's perceived influence on primary parent .76 In addition, the extent of the child's self-confidence is positively related to his use of television, magazines and movies (.60) and his use of mother, father, and relatives (.65) for ideas. Similarly, the extent to which a parent lets the child make his own decisions is positively related to the same sources, reSpectively (.62 and .53). In summary, it is noticeable with a few exceptions, the criterion measures were related to each other in the assumed direction. Actually, the child's indicated use of newspapers and peers for ideas were the only variables negatively correlated with the other criterion measures under study. Also, 69 the concept of parent control over the child and its relationship to other criterion measures could not be fully evaluated due to the lack of the in- ternal consistency of the scales. Relative predictive value of communication variables. The most con- sistent predictors of the child's behavior were the primary parent's sup- port of the child's ideas in a discussion and the lack of agreement shown by {-.16-n fl the child of his primary parent's ideas during the discussion. The relative ;! predictive value of each of these communication variables was also calculated. It was possible to partial out the influence attributable to the "non— primary parent's support of the child" in a discussion, the "child's support of his primary parent" in a discussion, and the "child's support of his non- primary parent" in a discussion, while checking on the relationship between the "primary parent's support of the child" in a discussion and the "child's perceived influence on his primary parent." This procedure was followed for each of the criterion measures under study. Only in the case of the child's perceived influence on his primary parent and the child's perceived selfeconfidence was it possible to compare the relative predictive value of the communication variables. The reason was that in regards to the primary parent's indicated control over the child and the child's claimed information-seeking, the primary parent's support of the child in a discussion was the only variable significantly related to these criterion measures. The data indicated that the primary parent's support of the child during the discussion consistently explained more of the variability in the child's behavior than did the lack of agreement shown by the child of his primary 70 Inuent's ideas in the discussion. The primary parent's support of the child in a discussion explained 21 per cent of the variability in the child's per— ceived influence on his primary parent, while the child's lack of agreement with his primary parent's ideas accounted for 18 per cent of the variability. Similarly, in explaining the child's perceived self-confidence, primary t parent's support of the child's ideas during the discussion accounted for j 26 per cent of the variability in this behavior, while the child's lack of agreement with his primary parent's ideas explained about 19 per cent. Furthermore, the primary parent's support of the child in a conversation explained #5 per cent of the variability in the primary parent's judgment whether to let the child make most of his own decisions. In addition, primary parent's support of the child's ideas during the discussion accounted for 29 per cent of the variability in the child's claimed use of television, magazines, and radio for ideas and about 16 per cent of the variability in the child's indicated use of his mother, father, and relatives as sources of information. Interrelationship of the_predictor measures. The initial assumption was that in a discussion, parental support of the child's ideas would be inversely related to the child's support of his parents' ideas (see page 38 for findings). The notion being, the more parents encourage their child to express his own ideas, the greater'the probability that the child's ideas will be in disagreement with that of his parents. A child that continually receives reinforcement from his parents should not feel reluctant to express mmtroversial ideas in a discussion with them. The data, however, point out somewhat of a different trend. The assumed 71 ./ inverse relationship holds between ,the child's support of his mother or primary parent and their support of him in a discussion. On the other hand, the relationship between the child's support of his father, and the mother or father's support of the child in a discussion is strongly positive. The original theoretical framework did not take into account that the child would relate to his parents in different ways, but these data strongly suggest a revision. Also, this could account for the reversed significant relationships in Hypotheses 2 and 11. The relationship between the concept of the child's contribution in a discussion and the concepts of parent and child support in a discussion would be difficult to analyze, since the data indicate that the range of former concept was restricted. The restricted range of the child's contribution in a discussion could be one factor which leads to lower correlations between it and the other predictors. The magnitude of the correlation coefficient varies with the degree of heterogeneity of the sample. Furthermore, one of the most important findings of this study was that neither the child's relationship with his mother or father explained the child's behavior with any consistency. Only the child's relationship with his primary parent seemed to be a regular predictor of the child's actions. Therefore, the Split into mother-child and father-child interaction patterns does not seem to be the most fruitful approach to the study of family communication. Generalizability of the study. The present study concerned itself with the process of building into a teen-agar the values that will enable him to depart from a family subsystem into the larger social system. To extricate himself from his irmnediate family, while teaching himself the norms of the larger social system he must move into, he must break a number of existing 72 dependencies and acquire a certain independence in his own thoughts and actions. The present study was designed to explore the patterns of communi- cation within his immediate family which might aid him in learning how to operate functionally in other social systems. In other words, this study focussed on the general process of training persons in the skills they need to know so that they can move from one system to another without much difficulty. They are trained to "get along" within the norms of the larger social system. The concern here, of course, is the process by which they learn how to overcome the narrow norms of their family subsystem, so that they can Operate effectively in other subsystems within the larger social system. This study dealt only with family communication. Whatever findings are borne out by this analysis, however, should apply to the communication behavior of persons in a wide variety of social situations. How strongly these findings apply to diverse social situations is subject to future research of course. However, it seems worth checking how strongly the socialization patterns designed to create perceived independence of thought and action in the family subsystem might alter a persons ability to adapt his behavior in other social systems. For example, laborers and foreman in small work groups, if they have management potential, tend to be trained to deve10p "independence" from the norms of their immediate subsystem so that they can manage persons to be productive in the goals of the larger social system. Through the process of communication a person acquires a level of independence that enables him to move with relative ease through social systems that vary in their norms and goals. ulna ,., y:- to: ‘ vb. .0” l,- -o u a ' v a '. -,.‘ u‘pu -|\~ ‘L! y'a- Ire. I.” ’J 73 Methodological Evaluations Better instrumentation. Several scale construction problems appeared. Four out of five hypotheses not supported (H: 3, 6, 9, 12) dealt with the percentage of the child's contribution to the total family discussion. If the family members contribute equally to the discussion, each member would have given one-third of the total contribution. Yet, one-third represented the upper-limit of the child's contribution, with a mean response rating of 20 percent. This restriction of the range could have been partially responsible fer the low correlations. However, it may not be reasonable to assume that a child could contribute equally with his parents in a discussion. If this be the case, the findings indicative virtually no relationship between the amount the child contributes in a discussion and the rest of the variables under study. Also, some scales that appeared in the questionnaire had reliability problems. Two of the hypotheses not supported (H: 8 and 9) included indices of parental control over the child. Two indices of control consisting of four items each, were constructed to measure the amount of control the mother and father exerted over their child. In both indices, one item correlated negatively with the other three, resulting in very weak measures. Further- more, the median correlations for the father and mother control indices were .ou and .32, respectively, indicating a lack of unidimensionality of the measures. Thus, the final analysis might have been confounded. Only in the case of the primary parent did the scales meet acceptable standards. Methodological alternatives. This study was limited to the extent that only one question was used to find out the child's preference for one parent or the other. In future studies, it might prove interesting to measure the 7'4 intensity of the child's feelings toward his parents. Also, instead of forcing the child to pick one "primary" parent over the other, he should have the alternative of selecting neither or pepp_parents as possible choices. Thus, there are the problems of giving the child more meaningful choice-alternatives and also determining how strongly he feels about the chosen parent. In addition, whether the child prefers the same parent across a number of different problem areas is another question which needs to be explored. As the child's problems change, so might his choice of primary parent, which suggests a need for a study which takes into account children at various ages and the problems they encounter at various stages of life. One of the reasons that only boys were included in this study was because girls do not tend to be confronted with the same problems that boys do for a given age. A concern for independence in boys, as earlier studies suggest, seems to take place at an earlier age than a similar concern on the part of girls. This is an empirical question, however, and needs further explication. Also, in order to verify the child's choice of primary parent, each parent should be asked the extent to which the child seeks information from either his mother or father. Actually, relationships outside of the immediate family, such as, peers and teachers should probably be taken into consideration since the basic conceptual notion deals with people who exert influence on the child. Are there distinct communication patterns between the child and his "primary" source of information? Discussion topics. Actual discussion time for the families ranged from 8 to 32 minutes, with an average (mean) time of 19 minutes. The "support patterns" constructed from the discussion, in some cases, could have been somewhat 75 restricted in variability. Theoretically, the potential range of the patterns was from +1 to -l, but none of them approached the limits. For example, primary parent support of the child ranged from +.u0 to -.30, with an average (mean) response of +.03. This pattern exhibited more variability than any of the others. Possibly, increased variability on the measures would be obtained by T. using a greater number of or more salient tOpics for discussion. I) Assumption of linearity. As mentioned previously, the zero-order correlations were checked to determine whether the assumption of linearity was reasonable. The data shown in Appendix F indicated that only a very few of the relationships might be suspect. On the whole, linearity did not seem to be a factor in the low correlations in this study. Sa_1_npling frame. Instead of the correlational approach used in this study, another type of analysis could have been performed on the data. When dealing with relationships between individuals and not the individual as the unit of analysis, a conditional probability analysis could be performed on the data. For example, the sequence of communicative utterances could be counted and the probability of who talks to whom and in what order could be estimated. With a mother, father, and a child in a discussion situation, the mother can either talk to her husband or the child, the father can talk to his wife or his child, and the child can either talk to his mother or father. Thus there are six possible combinations of communication sequences and when these are analyzed over an entire discussion, a "style" or a number of communication patterns could be established for that family. In a similar manner, it was the intent of this study to explore recurrent communication relationships between family members and the extent to which these relationships are common across families. 76 Contributions of the Study The evidence from this study suggests that family communication patterns are important in explaining a child's development. Sole reliance on the child's relationship with either his mother or father does not seem to point up any recurrent communication patterns. Only when the child's interaction with his primary parent is analyzed does a consistent pattern become apparent. The child who perceives himself as influencing his primary parent re- ceives encouragement and support of his ideas from that parent. This finding fellows from two theoretical propositions and one basic assumption. Costley (196%) indicates that the fewer non-supportive statements made by the group members, the more a member's personal attraction to the group. In addition, Cartwright and Zander (1953) state that attracted members are more likely to accept others' opinions and more often change their minds to take the views of fellow members. In order to tie the propositions together, it is necessary to assume that parents who make few non—supportive statements to their children will also be inclined to make more supportive statements. Therefore, the finding not only supports the hypothesis drawn from these propositions but adds greater specificity to the relationships within the family to which it applies. Both Gibb (1951) and Likert (1961) suggest that a democratic leadership style involves giving the group members ample opportunity to express their thoughts without being constrained by the leader pressing his own views. In the same manner, Douvan and Adelson (1966) report that democratic families tend to tolerate disagreement, while also having highly attracted members. As suggested before, attracted members are more accepting of other's Opinions and more often change their minds to take the views of fellow members. In this study, 77 the finding that the less the child supports his primary parent in a discussion, the more his perceived influence on this parent follows from the above prOpo- sitions. Douvan and Adelson furthermore suggest that children from democratic homes indicate a high level of participation in family decisions. Also, in a democratic family, support and participation are related to personal attraction; and attracted members more readily try to influence each other. However, from monitoring family discussion, the hypothesis that the more the child con- tributes to the discussion, the more the child will perceive that he influences his parents, is not confirmed. The rationale used in the development of the propositions dealing with a child's perceived influence is the same one used in explaining the development of a child's self-confidence. It has been shown that the child's perceived influence on his parents and his self-confidence are highly related. The findings suggest that the child who indicates that he is fairly self-confident in his own ability to make decisions also receives support of his ideas from his primary parent. Again, the child who can openly disagree with his primary parent seems to develop self-confidence in his own judgments. A parent who listens with attentiveness, communicates to his child that his ideas are valued and that he is respected. Such respect gives the child a sense of self-con- fidence. The feeling of personal worth might enable the child to deal more effectively with his own problems. Since perceived influence and self-confidence are suggested as important factors in the child's development, a similar rationale seems tenable in regards to parents' allowing their child to make most of his own decisions. Costley (1964) reports fewer non-supportive statements made by group members, the more .Q on. ‘ 5‘s .4‘ Ms" 1:; ¥ “in , V. y 78 the group members are satisfied with group decisions. Sargent (l96u) indicates that democratic leaders tend to make a high proportion of supportive statements, while phrasing most of their contributions in the ferm of questions, suggesting that parents with democratic values might indicate greater satisfaction with the child's decisions. The findings from this study indicate that primary parent support of the child in a discussion varies directly with the parent's judgment whether to let the child make hisown decisions. Unlike the relationship between a child's support of his primary parent in a conversation, and the child's perceived influence and self-confidence, the child's support of his primary parent in a conversation has only a slight influence on whether the parent lets the child make his own decisions. Similarly, the child's contribution to the family discussion has virtually no influence on the primary parent's decision. Finally, McLeod, Chaffee, and Wackman (1967) indicate that children from democratic homes are well informed and use the media for infermation. They also point out that democratic families encourage a child to explore new ideas and controversial material. The findings here indicate that support by the child's primary parent in a discussion has a direct bearing on the child's use of his mother, father, and relatives for ideas and his use of television, movies, and magazines as sources of information. As before, the child's support of his primary parent in a discussion is not related to his use of these personal and mediated sources for ideas. In addition, the child's interaction with his primary parent does not influence the child's claimed use of newspapers or peers as sources of ideas. Also, the amount the child contributes to the family discussion is not related to his use of these sources. 79 In the long-run the significance of these findings lies in the fact that usually the child observes the behavior of his parents and incorporates these behaviors into his own personality structure. The personality in the child develops slowly. A certain type of personality emerges or becomes stabilized to a degree by the interactive process of defining acts of others and thus becoming aware of one's own actions. This results in a persistent or stable pattern of behavior. Suggestions for Future Research In light of the findings of the exploratory study, attention now can be given to the consideration of future attempts in this direction. The findings dealing with the primary parent's relationship with the child seem to be a most fruitful area for further study. The concept of primary parent consistently aided in explaining the child's behavior. For example, primary parent support of the child during the discussion, explained u2 percent of the child's perceived influence on his primary parent. A question that presents itself is, what is the nature of the communi- cation between the child and his primary parent, regardless of parent's sex? Also, what other factors in the child's development are nourished by the child's interaction with this parent? A greater understanding of the child's preference for one parent or the other may be a promising area for communi- cation research. The group leader in the small group research may be compared with the concept of primary parent in the family, since both tend to encourage and support their members. However, the authority of the group leader stems from his position, whereas, this notion does not necessarily hold for the primary parent 80 This might lead one to ask about the relationship between family communication patterns and the nature of the power structure within the family. Also, the child's use of personal and mediated sources for information might be studied more closely. Why was the child's use of peers and news- papers for ideas negatively correlated with his use of the other sources? Are his use of television and movies a means of maintaining social relations within the family? Or better still, what are the functions served for the child by his use of various personal and mediated sources? It would be desirable to study a group of boys and girls over time as they move up to, and then through, one or more of these stages in their develop- nent. Not only should such a longitudinal study shed further light on this communication process and the factors involved, but it also would serve as a check on how well the present technique estimates the state of the relation- ships between family members. BIBLIOGRAPHY ‘ . Back, K. "Influence Through Social Communication," Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, No. #6 (1951), 9-23. Bales, R.F. Interaction Process Analysis. Cambridge, Mass.: Addison- Wesley, 1950. Burgess, E.W., and Locke, H.J. The Family From Institution to Companionship. New York: American Book Company, 1945. Cartwright, D. and Zander, A. Group Dynamics: Research and Theogy. Evanston, 111.: Row, Peterson and Company, 1953. Cohen, A.R. "Experimental Effects of Ego-Defense Preferences on Interpersonal Relations," Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, No. 52 (1956), 19-27. Costley, Dan Lanier. "A Study of the Relationships Between Selected Factors in Interpersonal Communication and Group Attraction." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 196%. Douvan, Elizabeth, and Adelson, Joseph. EmergLing Conceptual Frameworks in Family Analysis. New York: The MacMillan Company, 1966. Festinger, L.; Pepitone, A.; and Newcomb, T. "Some Consequences of De-individuation in a Group," Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, No. #7 (1952), 382-389. Festinger, L.; Schachter, 8.; and Back, K. Social Pressures in Informal Gropps. New York: Harper, 1950. Gerard, H.B. "The Effects of Two Dimensions of Disagreement on the Influence Process in Small Groups," Human Relations, No. 6 (1953), 2u9-271. ) . Gibb, C.A. "An Experimental Approach to the Study of Leadership," Occupational Psychology, No. XXV (1951), 233-2u8. Hall, Calvin S.; and Hill, Rueben. The Family: A Dynamic Interpretation. New York: John Wiley 8 Sons, Inc., 1957. Hill, Rueben, and Hansen, Donald A. "The Identification of Conceptual Frameworks Employed in Family Study," Marriage and Family Living, No. 22 (November, 1960), 299-311. Huntington, Robert M. "New Approaches in Family Research: A Symposium; the Personality-Interaction Approach to Study of Marital Relationship," Marriage and Family Living, No. 20 (February, 1958), "’3" 1+6 0 81 82 Kenkel, William F. The Family in Perspective. 2nd ed. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1961. Kirkpatrick, Clifford. The Family: As Process and Institution. New York: 7 \ The Ronald Press Company, 1945. Iewin, K.; Lippitt, R.; and White, R.K. ‘"Patterns of Aggressive Behavior in Experimentally Created 'Social Climates'," Journal of Social Psycholggy, No. X (1939), 271-299. Iikert,Rensis. New Patterns of Management. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961. McLeod, Jack M.; Chaffee, Steven H.; and Wachman, Daniel B. "Family Communication: An Updated Report," Paper presented to the Theory and Methodology Division Association for Education in Journalism, University of Wisconsin, 1967.: i r I! McNemar, Quinn. Psychological StatiStics. 3rd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1949, p. 167. _g g '1‘ , ,., Nye, F. Ivan; and Berardo, Felix M. Eme in Conce tual Frameworks in Family Analysis. New York: ‘Free Press of Glencoe, Inc., 1955. Riley, Matilda White; and Riley, John W. Jr. "A Sociological Approach to Communications Research," Public Opinion Quarterly, (1951), 445-460. Rogers, Carl R. ‘Qn Becoming a Person: A Therapist's View of Psycho- therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1961. Sargent, James Franklin. "Differences in Communication Behaviors of Authoritarian and Democratic Leaders," Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1964. Schachter, 8. "Deviation, Rejection, and Communication," Journal of Abnormal Social.P§ycholpgy, No. 46 (1951), 190-207. Strodtbeck, Fred L. "Family Interaction, Values and Achievement," Talent and Sociegy. Princeton, N.J.: Van Nostrand Company, Inc., 1958. Stryker, Sheldon. "The Interactional and Situational Approaches," Handbook of Marriage and the Family. Chicago: Rand McNally 8 Company, 1964, 125-170. "Symbolic Interaction as an Approach to Family Research," Marrigge and FamilyLiving, No. 21 (May, 1959), 111-119. Terman, Lewis M. Esychological Factors in Marital Happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc., 1938. Thomas, E. and Burdick, H. "Self-esteem and Defense Preference as Related to Social Behavior." Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan, 1954. Truxal, Andrew G.; and Merrill, Francis E. Marriage and the Family. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1953. 83 Vogel, Ezra R.; and Bell, Norman W. "The Emotionally Disturbed Child as the Family Scapegoat," A Modern Introduction to the_Family. New York: Free Press of Glencoe, Inc., 1960, 382-397. \ Waller, Willard; and Hill, Rueben. The Family: A Dynamic Interpretation, rev. ed. New York: The Dryden Press, Inc., 1951. Winch, Robert F. Mate Selection: A Study of Complimentary Needs. New York: Harper 8 Row, Pub., 1958. White, R.K.; and Lippitt, R. Autocracy and Democra_y. New York: Harper 8 Harper, 1960. tin-av .z. . APPENDIX A LETTER SENT TO RBSPONDENTS 85 July, 1968 Dear Mr. 8 Mrs. Most of us recognize that what we say to others is pretty important. We are interested in the important communication within families; that is, what do families talk about and how do they talk about certain tapics. We would, therefore, like to study the communication which goes on between members of a family. meq E We are graduate students in the Department of Communication at Michigan State University. Our interests are in looking at patterns of discussion within families as they relate to other kinds of social activity, such as, where you go to look for ideas about particular topics, and how you go about making decisions about things of concern to the family. ~-,_ Your family (father, mother, and teen-age son) would be asked to participate I by sitting and discussing a few topics, such as school activities, dating, "“' and so forth. This discussion would take about one half hour followed by a short questionnaire concerning how you make the daily decisions of concern to your family. You can be assured that the answers of any specific family will not be made public. We are interested in groups of families only, and in their patterns of discussion. In two or three days we will be contacting you by phone to ask for your c00peration in discussing among yourselves, topics concerned with areas of interest to you and your family. We hope that you will agree to help us in our graduate programs. Thank you for your time in reading this letter. We hope that you will be interested in our research and find an hour in your day in which to help .us. Sincerely , Daniel E. Costello Duane D. Pettersen Graduate Assistant Graduate Assistant APPENDIX B DEFINITIONS OF INTERACTION ANALYSIS CATEGORIES 87 I. POSITIVE SOCIALPEMOTIONAL REACTIONS Acts of Active Solidarity and Affection. Includes greeting another kw saying "hello" or in some other friendly manner, welcoming another, was in return to a friendly gesture, accepting an offer of help or assistance, thanking another, indications of mannerly considerations, indications of good will, indications that the actor is friendly. A :fidendly comment on the weather or some other matter of common interest n>"break the ice." The expression of sympathy - "I can see how you feel." Any indication in the course of interaction that the relationship is becoming intimate or familiar. Any act of adherence where the actor chooses to be a fellow member with another. Status-Raising Acts. Includes all acts which have the specific afim or effect of raising or enhancing the other's status. Including praising, rewarding, boosting the other, giving approval or encouragement. Examples: "That's fine," "That's a good idea," "Swell," "You've covered a lot of ground today." Complimenting, congratulating showing approval of another, giving credit to another, showing enthusiasm for another's _views. Expressing gratitude or appreciation, showing admiration or respect. Responses to Shows of Tension. Includes any behavior in which the actor offers assistance to another. Any act of sharing. Any behavior in which the actor defends another. Giving support, reassurance, comfort, encouragement, showing of sympathy. Responses to Disagreements. Includes acts which may appear after a situation of difficulty, such as interceding, mediating, or moderating in a difficulty between two others. Any act where the actor urges unity or harmony, agreement, cooperation, or expressed other values of solidarity. 88 In cases of disagreement or antagonism between members, the suggesting of a compromise. Indications of Relief. Includes exPressions of fieling better after a period of tension, any manifestation of cheerfulness, satisfaction, enjoyment, pleasure, delight, joy, happiness. Positive responses to a compliment. Includes the making of friendly jokes, trying to amuse or entertain. Positive responses to joking, such as smiling, grinning, or chuckling. Regaonses to Acts of Decision with Agreement. Includes any con- currence in a proposed course of action. Examples: "I second the motion," "Let's do that." Includes any act in which the actor either verbally or overtly complies with a request or suggestion. Agreement with an obser- vation or report, or analysis which another has made. Examples: "That's the way I see it too," "I think you are right about that," "Yes, that's true." Similarly includes agreement, approval, or endorsement of an ex- pression of value or feeling. Examples: "I feel the same way you do ," "I hope so too," "That's right." Includes giving any sign of recognition, interest, receptiveness, readiness, reaponsiveness. Includes giving specific signs of attention to what the other is saying by nodding or saying "I see," "Yes." Includes showing comprehension, understanding, or insight. Examples: "Oh," "I see," "Yes ," "Sure, now I get it." Includes admitting an error or oversight, admitting that some objection or disapproval is valid, conceding a point to the other, giving way, withdrawing politely. Examples: "Now I may be wrong about this. . ." "This is not an important point perhaps . . ." Includes any indication of a permissive attitude, where another is led to understand that he is accepted "as he is ," so that the incorrectness of his solution to any problem or the quality of his performance does not 89 adversely affect his status, so that he can "make mistakes without blame." Includes any act in which the actor submits passively, accepts coercion, criticism, without retaliation, rebuttal, rebellion, or complaint. II. ATTEMPTED ANSWERS: TASK AREA Giving Suggestions or Direction Related to Task. Includes all acts which suggest concrete ways of attaining a desired goal by attacking or modifying the outer situation, or by adapting activity to it, proposing a solution, indicating or suggesting where to start, what to do, how to c0pe with a problem in terms of action in the near future. Includes giving instructions or making proposals, showing where, when, how, why, something is to be done. Examples: "We will have to stop at the end of one-half hour." "Consider for a moment what would happen if..." "Suppose we set up the following situation..." "Go right ahead." Includes direct attempts to guide the others regarding some activity, to persuade someone, to urge or to inspire someone. Giving Opinions, Evaluations, or Analysis. Includes all indications of thought-in-process leading to an understanding, such as reasoning, thinking, or concentrating. The actual statement of a hypothesis or ex- pression of understanding or insight. Includes logical elaboration, exploration, or testing of a hypothesis, whether by example, analogy, analysis of cause and effect relations, categorical labeling or any sort of conjectural process. Includes any expression of desire, want, liking, wishing, any expression of moral obligation, any affirmation of values, any statement of intention, referring to a broad and indefinite future time perspective, as yet unimplemented as to ways and means. Examples: "I think we ought to be fair about this." "I hope we can do something about that." "That seems to be the right thing to do." Includes activity T’“”‘ 1%,,” A . A) “‘zflie. 90 in which the actor attempts, by inference or reasoning, to understand or interpret his own motivation or the "why" of his own behavior in relation to the prdblem being discussed. Examples: "I can see now that I mis- judged the situation." "I think I behave that way because..." Includes activity in which the actor attempts to understand the motivation or activities of others in relation to the problem situation. Includes all T"fi§ statements about the nature of the outer situation in relation to the nnf group. Giving Orientation, Information or Clarifying, Includes all acts which are intended to focus attention on the problem to be discussed, calling attention to what one is going to say, or pointing out the relevance of what one is saying. Examples: "There are two points I'd like to make." "In the first place..." "Now with regard to our problem of..." "Going back for a moment..." "What I am about to say relates to..." Includes efforts to prevent or repair breaks in the flow of communication, such as, repeating, clarifying confusion about something said, explaining, summarizing, restating. Includes any account of one's own private experience where the actor tells what he felt, what was done, how it was done, the position he took on some issue. Includes showing an understanding of the other or something the other has said by restating or reporting the essential content of what has been said. Includes statements of fact about the nature of the outer situation facing the group. III. QUESTIONS: TASK AREA Asking for Orientation of Information. Includes acts which indicate or express a lack of knowledge, confusion or uncertainty about the position of the group with regard to its task, about what has been said or is going 91 on, about the meaning of a word or phrase. Includes the appearance of any attitude the observer would describe as puzzled, bewildered, or baffled. Examples: "What?" "What was that?" "I don't quite get what you mean." "Where are we?" "Where do we stand now?" Includes direct or outright questions which require the giving of a factual answer. Also includes more indefinite expressions of a lack of knowledge. Examples: "I don't know about this." "It isn't clear to me." "It may be true, or it may not be." ASE£2§,f°P Opinion, Evaluation, or Analysis. Includes any kind of question which attempts to encourage a statement or reaction on the part of another without limiting the nature of the response. Examples: "Tell me more about it." "Tell me more about..." "What do you think?" "What should our policy be?" Includes inferences or evaluations requested. Examples: "How long do you suppose it will be?" "I can't figure out how long it would take." "I wonder if there are any other possibilities?" "Why do you think you feel that way?" Asking for Suggestions or Direction. Includes requests for suggestions as to what should be done in terms of finding ways, means, and solutions, requests for suggestions as to where to start, what to do next, what to decide. Examples: "I wonder what we can do about this?" "I don't know what to do." "What do you suggest?" IV. NEGATIVE SOCIALPEMOTIONAL REACTIONS Disagree or Shows Passive Rejection. Includes passive forms of re- jection such as remaining immobile, rigid, silent, uncommunicative, responseless, in the face of overtures of others. Working at something other than the problem with which the group is concerned. Includes 92 disagreement, disbelief, incredulity regarding reports and observations made by others. Includes failure to give requested repetition, ignoring a request of any kind or a complaint. Examples: "I don't think so." "I'm not going to repeat it." Showing Tension, Asks for Help, Includes all manifestations of impatience, indications that the subject feels strained, on edge, restless, agitated. Includes any manifestation or indication to the observer that the actor is startled, alarmed, dismayed, or has misgivings about something he has done or intends to do. Any show of anxious emotionality, such as lesitation, speechlessness, trembling, blushing, stammering. Includes nervous or apologetic acts where actor admits his own ignorance or incapacity. Acts of blaming, belittling, accusing, condemning, scorning, humiliating. Includes expressions of unhappiness, discouragement, despair, brooding, distress, discomfort, fatigue. Showing any kind of need to be supported, forgiven, consoled. Includes any behavior which indicates that the actor is unattentive, bored, or psychologically withdrawn - slouching, yawning, daydreaming. ShowinglAntaggnism. Includes the arbitrary assignment of a role, a defining or restricting of another's power by demands or commands such as "Come here!" "Stop that!" "Hurry up!" "Get out!" Includes acts that are assertive, inconsiderate, repressive. Includes any act in which the actor rejects, refuses, or ignores directions, shrugging the shoulders, avoiding or quitting activities. Includes aggressive acts such as griping, nagging, annoying, disturbing, or pestering others. Includes attempts to override the other in conversation, interrupting the other. Active attacks on another's status, any implication of inferiority or incompetence on the part of another. Includes making charges against another, blaming, imputing unworthy motives, denouncing. Includes any acts of disapproval 93 of self, or of others. Includes any behavior in which the actor appears to be provoking or irritating. Includes threats, attacking and challenging others. APPENDIX C DISCUSSION QUESTIONS 95,, Below are a series of situations which are discussed in most families at one time or another. Families seem to handle the problems in different ways. We would like for you to discuss among yourselves as many points of view that you are familiargwith. From these different views, select a view which represents the thinking of your family. Please spend some time with each of the situations. There are no right or wrong answers...only what your family feels is the most appropriate answer for the situation. You will have 30 minutes to discuss all four situations below. Don't worry about the time; we will inform you when the half hour is up. (1) It has been proposed that the minimum age for getting a drivers' license be raised from age 16 to age 18. Discuss the different points of view that you are familiar with on this topic, and select one which represents the thinking of your family. (2) Some parents feel that if their teen-age son or daughter wants to smoke, the choice should be left up to the teen-ager. Other parents insist that their teen-age son or daughter may smoke only when the parents consider them ready to smoke. Discuss the different points of view that you are familiar with on this tapic, and select one which represents the thinking of your family. (3) Some parents allow their teen-age son or daughter to go to movies based on violence, sex, and other adult themes. Other parents regard films recommended for adults only as strictly off-limits, until the teen ager is older and more mature in his thinking. Discuss the different points of view that you are familiar with on this topic, and select one which represents the thinking of your family. (4) Some parents feel that their teen-age son or daughter should be allowed to wear their hair or clothes in keeping with the current fads. Other parents insist that conformity to teen age fads is unnecessary and that teen-agers should leave the final decision up to their'parents. Discuss the different points of view that you are familiar with on this topic, and select one which represents the thinking of your family. 97 CHI LD' S QUEST IONNAI RE _I To begin with...what specific topics ”are discussed m_o_s_t often in your family at meal-time? I‘d like you to read each of the following questions carefully...then place an "X" in the blank in front of the answer you consider most appropriate. Please choose only one answer for your father...and gn_e_ answer for your mother for each ques_t-i-0n. 11-12 About how often would you say your parents ask you for your opinion on family decisions? L j; MOTHER FATHER Several times a week Several times a week About once a week _About once a week —Once or twice a month —Once or twice a month :Less than once a month :Less than once a month 13-14 If your parents said that they depended a great deal on your judgment regarding family decisions, would you believe them? MOTHER FATHER Yes Yes I guess so --_I guess so :Probably not :Probably not ___No ___NO 15-16 Compared with other teen-agers...are you more likely, or less likely... to be asked by your parents for Opinions on family decisions? MOTHER FATHER _More likely _More likely :Less likely :Less likely __About the same :About the same 17-18 In regards to family decisions, would you like to think your parents consider your opinions? MOTHER FATHER __Yes _Yes __I guess so :I guess so __Probably not :Probably not No —No 97 CHI LD' S QUESTIONNAIRE in! To begin with...what specific topics are discussed m_o_s_t Often in your family at meal-time? I'd like you to read each of the following questions carefully...then place an"X" in the blank in front of the answer you consider most appropriate. Please choose only one answer for your father. .and O_n_e answer for your mother for each quest1on. ll-12 About how Often would you say your parents ask you for your Opinion on family decisions? MOTHER FATHER Several times a week _Several times a week _About once a week _About once a week Once or twice a month Once or twice a month :Less than once a month :Less than once a month 13-14 If your parents said that they depended a great deal on your judgment regarding family decisions, would you believe them? MOTHER FATHER _Yes _Yes —I guess 80:1 guess so _Probably not :Probably not ___No ___No 15-16 Compared with other teen-agers...are you more likely, or less likely... to be asked by your parents for Opinions on family decisions? MOTHER FATHER _More likely _More likely :Less likely :Less likely _About the same :About the same 17-18 In regards to family decisions, would you like to think your parents consider your Opinions? MOTHER FATHER _Yes Yes _I guess so :1 guess so __Probably not :Probably not _No :NO 98 19-20 When you discuss family decisions with your parents, what part do you usually play? MOTHE R FATHER I talk mostly I talk mostly I mainly listen I mainly listen A little of both A little of both Now, I would like for you to read about some problems that teen-agers your age sometimes face. Listed below are some of the possible ways of finding answers to these problems. If you were concerned about finding a part-time job and uncertain as to what choice to make, how often would you look for ... OFTEN NOW 8 THEN SELDOM NEVER 21. IDEAS for newspapers 22. IDEAS from your mother 23. IDEAS from television 24. IDEAS from others your age 25. IDEAS from magazines 26. IDEAS from your father 27. IDEAS from movies 28. IDEAS from other relatives If you needed to buy clothes for school and uncertain as to what choice to make, how often would you look for ... OF'I‘EN NOW 5 THEN SE LDOM NEVER 29. IDEAS from newspapers 30. IDEAS from your mother 31. IDEAS from television 32. IDEAS from others your age 33. IDEAS from magazines 34. IDEAS from your father 35. IDEAS from movies 36. IDEAS from other relatives 37. On matters such as jobs and clothing, if you could ask for ideas from only one of your parents, which parent would you ask? Mother Father 98L Ikw here is a different kind of question. Place an "X" in the blank in :fixmt of the answer'you consider most apprOpriate. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. l«‘3. an, 45. 47. If you had just bought new clothes for school, how sure would you be that you had made the best choice possible? Very sure Sure Not sure If you had just joined a new club at school, how sure would you be that you had made the best choice possible? Very sure Sure Not sure If you had just accepted a part-time jcb for this summer, how sure would you be that you had made the best choice possible? Very sure Sure Not sure Finally...just a few more questions about yourself...what is your age? And what was the last grade you completed in school? Are you taking or going to take a college prep or vocational or business training courses in high school? College prep. Vocational Business What Subject is easiest for you? What subject is hardest for you? For the first 10 years of your childhood, what state or country did you live in for most of these years? Did you live primarily in a rural (farm) or urban (city) area during this time? rural urban Do you speak a language other than English? Yes NO What language ? Does anyone else in your family Speak it? Mother Father Is it used frequently in the home? Yes NO APPENDIX D MOTHER'S QUESTIONNAIRE 100 To begin with...what specific topics are discussed most often in your family at meal-time? I'd like you to read each Of the following questions carefully...then place an "X" in the blank in front of the answer you consider most appropriate. Please choose only one answer for each question. . In general, how are most decisions made between you and your teenage son on the following Situations . . . 11. 12. 13. 14. If your son needed new clothes for school...does he usually choose his own, or do you decide for him? I always decide for him I often decide for him Every now and then I decide for him I seldom decide for him I never decide for him In regards to your son's friends...does he usually choose his own, or do you suggest who they should be? I always suggest to him I Often suggest to him Every now and then I suggest to him I seldom suggest to him I never suggest to him When your son goes out with others his own age..does he usually come home when he wants to, or do you usually remind him of what time to be home? I always remind him I often remind him Every now and then I remind him I seldom remind him I never remind him In regards to your son's dating...does he usually determine how often he goes out, or do you tell him when he is allowed to date? I always tell him I often tell him Every now and then I tell him I seldom tell him I never tell him 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 101 Finally...a few questions about yourself...what is your age? And what was the last grade you completed in school or college? How many children do you have living? What are their ages? Does anyone else live with your family? Who? Do you speak a language other than English? Yes No What language ? Does anyone else in your family speak it? Husband Teen-age son Is it used frequently in the home? Yes No For the first 10 years of your childhood, what state or country did you live in for most of these years? Did you live primarily in a rural (farm) or urban (city) area during this time? rural urban What is your family's religion? Protestant Catholic Jewish Other I APPENDIX E FATHER'S QUESTIONNAIRE 103 To begin with...what Specific tOpics are discussed most often in your family at meal-time? I'd like you to read each of the following questions carefully...then place an "X" in the blank in front of the answer you consider most appropriate. Please choose only one answer for each question. In general, how are most decisions made between you and your teenage son on the following situations. . . 11. 12. 13. 14. If your son needed new clothes for school...does he usually choose his own, or do you decide for him? I always decide for him I often decide for him Every now and then I decide for him I seldom decide for.him I never decide for him In regards to your son's friends...does he usually choose his own, or do you suggest who they should be? I always suggest to him I often suggest to him Every now and then I suggest to him I seldom suggest to him I never suggest to him When your son goes out with others his own age...does he usually come home when he wants to, or do you usually remind him of what time to be home? I always remind him ' I often remind him Every now and then I remind him I seldom remind him I never remind him In regards to your son's dating...does he usually determine how Often he goes out, or do you tell him when he is allowed to date? I always tell him I often tell him Every now and then I tell him I seldom tell him I never tell him 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 104 Finally...a few questions about yourself...what is your age? And what was the last grade you completed in school or college? What kind Of work do you do or usually do? What type of business or industry do you work for? What was your family's approximate total annual income for last year (1967)? DO you Speak a language other than English? Yes No What language ? Does anyone else in your family Speak it? Wife Teen-age son Is it used frequently in the home? Yes No For the first 10 years of your childhood, what state or country did you live in fOr most of these years? Did you live primarily in a rural (farm) or urban (city) area during this time? rural urban APPENDIX F TABLE 21 Table 21. A check on the assunption of rectilinearity: 106 Pearson product-moment r with Eta Comparison of the Criterion Predictor 2 2 Variables Variables r Eta INFLUENCE INDEX Child's influence on Mother's support .40 .46 mother... Father's support .04 .05 Support of mother .05 .10 Support of father .15 .26 Child's contribution .08 .17 Child's influence on Mother's support .23 .47 father. . . Father' s , support . 0 8 . 16 Support of mother .02 .13 Support of father .03 .20 Child's contribution .02 .11 Child's influence on Primary parent support .66 .67 primary parent... Non-primary parent support .02 .18 Support of primary parent .12 .12 Support of non-primary parent .27 .32 Child's contribution .03 .10 Child's influence on Primary parent support .49 .52 non-primary parent... Non-primary parent support .01 .07 Support of primary parent .02 .03 Support of non-primary parent .23 .38 Child's contribution .07 .20 SELF-CONFIDENCE INDEX Child's self-confidence Mother's support .44 .56 in his own judgments... Father's support .10 .10 Primary parent support .55 .71 Non-primary parent support .14 .23 Support of mother .14 .26 Support of father .01 .04 Support of primary parent .21 .23 Support of non-primary parent .12 .17 Child's contributions .00 .06 107 Criterion Predictor 2 2 Variables Variables r Eta CONTROL INDEX Mother's control over Mother's support .04 .08 child... Father's support .05 .13 Support of mother .00 .18 Support of father .11 .20 Child's contribution .00 .00 Father's control over Mother's support .09 .15 child... Father's support .06 .08 Support of mother .00 .23 Support of father .08 .09 Child's contribution .00 .01 Primary parent control Primary parent support .51 .51 over child... Non-primary parent support .05 .25 Support of primary parent .09 .09 ' Support of non-primary parent .20 .27 Child's contribution , .01 .07 Non-primary parent control Primary parent support .00 .02 over child... 7 Non-primary parent support .00 .10 Support of primary parent .02 .03 Support of non-primary parent .01 .07 Child's centribution .01 .09 INFORMATION-SEEKING INDEX Child's use of Mother's support .03 .07 newspapers... Father's support .00 .08 Primary parent support .08 .10 Non-primary parent support .01 .04 Support of mother ‘ .04 .13 Support of father .01 .03 Support of primary parent .06 .06 Support of non-primary parent .02 .10 Child's contribution .00 .03 108 Criterion Predictor Variables Variables r2 Eta Child's use of magazines, Mother's support .08 .22 television, and movies... Father's support .12 .18 Primary parent support .18 .23 Non-primary parent support .00 .10 Support of mother .07 .12 Support of father .01 .15 Support of primary parent .15 .16 Support of non-primary parent .02 .05 Child's contribution .02 .05 Child's use of persons Mother's support .02 .05 his own' age. . .. Father's support .08 .14 Primary parent support .00 .20 Non-primary parent support .01 .22 Support of mother .03 .14 Support of father .00 .04 Support of primary parent .00 .00 Support of non-primary parent .03 .05 Child's contribution .04 .06 Child's use of mother, Mother's support .18 .36 father, and relatives... Father's support .09 .10 Primary parent support .45 .48 Non-primary parent support .02 .13 Support Of mother .01 .03 Support of father .03 .18 Support of primary parent .02 .03 Support of non-primary parent .20 .33 Child's contribution .03 .19 "Immm