PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or boron date duo. ‘ a DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LINEAR ORDER IN HAYA VERBAL MORPHOLOGY: THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS By Yunus Ismail Rubanza A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Linguistics and Germanic, Slavic, Asian and African Languages 1988 r 0'“ be H"? ABSTRACT LINEAR ORDER OF THE HAYA VERBAL MORPHEMES: THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS By Yunus Ismail Rubanza This study examined the factors which determine the linear order and the cooccurrence restrictions of the Haya verbal. morphemesn .Also the study' discussed these determining factors SUI relation 1x) Bybee’s (1985) theory, which claims a correlation of a degree of semantic relatedness of the inflectional categories to the verb-stem, and the formal structure of the verb. The study is divided into four parts: (1) A phonological sketch of the Haya language, (2) identification of the verb morphemes and their functions, (3) an account, where possible, for the cooccurrence restrictions of verbal morphemes, and (4) the relation of the linear order and the cooccurrence restrictions to B’s theory of RELEVANCE and GENERALITY. . The data for this study comes from Haya books,l newspapers, transcripts of tape recordings and from the author, a first language speaker of Haya. The major findings of this study are: (1) the Haya language has twenty three verbal morphemes, of which only Yunus Ismail Rubanza twenty two are examined in this study. However, not all of these morphemes do exhibit the maximum degree of cooccurrence with each other. (2) Four factors are identified as responsible for the cooccurrence restrictions of the verbal morphemes. These factors are: phonological, syntactic, semantic and pragmatic. (3) The linear order of the Haya verbal morphemes , supports B’ s predictions concerning hierarchy and order and more generally RELEVANCE and GENERALITY. The findings of this study ‘bring to the fore the observation that a model of the linear order of the verbal morphemes of Haya (and this may be true of other languages) must consider not only the semantic relevance of the morphemes to the verbs but also other aspects of linguistics, for example, phonology, syntax, and'pragmatics. This work is dedicated to my late parents: Ismail Rubanza and Halima Abdallahmani-Rubanza. iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to express my sincere gratitude to Dr. Carolyn Harfordq vdu) as guidance Committee Chairperson and dissertation director provided support, ~ insight, and challenge. Dr. Harford was there when needed, and provided perspectives which kept this study within manageable limits. I would also like to thank my doctoral committee: Dr. Ruth Brend, Dr. Barbara Arbbott, Dr. David Dwyer, Dr. Seok C. Song and Dr. Denise Troutman-Robinson. Acknowledgements must also be extended to former guidance committee Chairpersons: Dr. Carol Scotton and Dr. Grover Hudson who provided guidance during coursework, comprehensive exams and the early stages of this dissertation. I would also like to thank the Chairpersons: Dr. D. Mkude, Prof. J. Wang and Dr. T. Juntune for support and encouragement. A dept of gratitude is owed to Dr. D. Wiley, Dr. Y. Fisseha and the staff of the African Studies Center and Department of Linguistics and Germanic, Slavic, Asian and African Languages for financial support and supplies. I also wish to express my deep appreciation to many who contributed to this project: to University of Dar— es-Salaam, Michigan State University for tuition and fees, Smithsonian Institution and Sage foundation for research and producing dissertation respectively; to Dr. Peter Seitel for data and guidance, Dr. and Mrs Rugumamu S. for offering me a place to stay during research implementation in Washington; V to Dr. Kenneth Ebert of International Students and Scholars Office for administrative support; Fr. Deogratias Mutefunya, Fr and Dr. Peter Rutoyongororwa, Dr. John Rutayuga for providing me with Haya books, newspapers and guidance; my friends Robert Mabagala, Dr. Kulikoyela Kahigi, Julie Chamberlain, Steve Burghess, my students; and the Michigan State Community who helped make my difficult days seem bearable. Finally, a very special dept of gratitude is owed to my wife Amida and my children Halima, Hashimu and Farida who lived tolerantly through the long days of studies and writing. I greatly aknowledge a dept of love, support and caring. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES D O O O O O C O O I I 0 LIST OF SYMBOLS AND ABBREVIATIONS. . . CHAPTER 1: BACKGROUND INFORMATION. CHAPTER 2: 1.1. Goals. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1.2. The Language . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1.2.1. Classification . . . . . . . . . . . 1.2.2. Dialects of Haya . . . . . . . . . . 1.3. The People . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1.4. Publications and data. . . . . . . . . 1.4.1. Published material . . . . . . . . . 1.5. Organisation of dissertation . . . . . Chapter 1: Endnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . PHONOLOGICAL SKETCH OF HAYA . . . . . 2.1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2. Phonological Inventory . . . . . . . . 2.2.1. Inventory of Consonant Phonemes. . . 2.2.1.1. Distinctive Features of Consonant Phonemes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2.1.2. Redundancy Rules for Consonant Phonemes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2.'. Inventory of Vowel Phonemes. . . . . 2.2.2.1. Distinctive Features of Vowel Phonemes . . . . . . . . . . vowels O O O 0 O O O O O 0 O O O 0 O 0 vii 2.2.2.2. Redundancy Rules for Vowel Phonemes. 2.3. The Tones. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.3.1. Lexical Tone . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.3.2. Grammatical Tone . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4. Distribution of Consonant and Vowel Phonemes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4.1. Distribution of Consonants . . . . . . 2.4.1.1. Distribution of Consonants Occurring Singly . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4.1.2. Consonant Clusters . . . . . . . . . 2.4.2. Distribution of Vowels . . . . . . . . 2.4.2.1. Short Vowels . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4.2.2. Long Vowels. . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.4.3. Distribution of Consonant Before Page xii xiii Hood-dozmwan-a H 12 13 13 16 Page 2.5. Phonological Rules . . . . . . . . . . . 33 2.5.1. Phonological Rules Affecting Consonants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 2.5.1.1. Nasal Assimilation . . . . . . . . . 33 2.5.1.2. Consonant Alternation. . . . . . u . 35 2.5.1.2.1. Strengthening. . . . . . . . . . . 35 2.5.1.2.2. Spirantization . . . . . . . . . . 37 2.5.1.2.3. Palatilization . . . . . . . . . . 39 2.5.1.2.4. Morphophonemic Rules . . . . . . . 40 2.5.2. Phonological Rules Affecting Vowels. . 42 2.5.2.1. Glide Formation. . . . . . . . . . . 42 2.5.2.2. Vowel Deletion . . . . . . . . . . . 44 2.5.2.3. Vowel Lengthening. . . . . . . . . . 45 2.5.2.3.1. Lengthening of Identical Vowel Sequence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 2.5. 2.3. 2. Lengthening before Nasal Clusters. 46 2.5. 2.3. 3. Lengthening before n . . . . . . . 47 2.6. A Word on Syllable Structure . . . . . . 48 2.7. General Remarks and Summary. . . . . . . 51 Chapter 2: Endnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 CHAPTER 3: THE MORPHEMES OF THE VERB FORMS AND FUNCTIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 3.1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 3.2. The Root . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 3. 3. The Non-Stem Morphemes . . . . . . . . . 60 3.3.1. The Indicative Morpheme. . . . . . . . 60 3.3.2. The Infinitive Morpheme. . . . . . . . 61 3.3.3. The Subject Marker . . . . . . . . . . 63 3.3.3.1. The Noun-Class System. . . . . . . . 63 3.3.4. The Subjunctive Morpheme . . . . . . . 68 3.3.5. The Tenses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 3.3.5.1. The Present Tenses . . . . . . . . . 71 3.3.5.1.1. Present Universal Habitual . . . . 72 3.3.5.1.2. "Still" Tense. . . . . . . . . . . 73 3.3.5.2. The Past Tenses. . . . . . . . . . . 74 3.3.5.2.1. Immediate Past . . . . . . . . . . 74 3.3.5.2.2. Near Past. . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 3.3.5.2.3. Recent Past. . . . . . . . . . . . 76 3.3.5.2.4. Remote Past. . . . . . . . . . . . 77 3.3.5.2.5. Far Remote Past. . . . . . . . . . 79 3.3.5.2.6. Immediate Past Habitual. . . . . . 80 3.3.5.3. The Future Tenses. . . . . . . . . . 81 3.3.5.3.1. Immediate Future . . . . . . . . . 82 3.3.5.3.2. Near Future. . . . . . . . . . . . 85 3.3.5.3.3. Remote Future. . . . . . . . . . . 86 3.3.5.3.4. Immediate Future Habitual. . . . . 88 3.3.5.4. The "Imaginary" -ku- Tense . . . . . 90 3.3.6. The Subject relative morpheme. . . . . 91 3.3.7. The Conditional Morpheme . . . . . . . 95 3.3.8. The Focus Morpheme . . . . . . . . . . 97‘ 3.3.9. The Negative Morphemes . . . . . . . . 103 viii 1. NEGl . . . . . . . . . . 2. NEG2 . . . . . . . . . . 3. NEG3 . . . . . . . . . . The Suffixal Morphemes. . 0.1. The Locative Morphemes. 0.2. The Manner Morpheme . . 0.3. The Intensifier Morpheme 0.4. The Question Morphemes. The Stem Morphemes . . . . . . The Object Marker. . . . . The Reflexive Marker . . The Verbal Extensions. . . The Causative Extension. The Applied Extension. . The Passive Extension. . The Reciprocal Extension The Repetitive Extension The Stative Extension. . . Conclusion . . . . . . . . Chapter 3: Endnotes . . . . . . wwwwwmwwwwwwawwwwww C C. O. O .0 O O . O O 0.. C C . CHAPTER 4: THE MORPHEMES OF THE VERB: RESTRICTIONS. O O I O O O O I O O O O 4.1. Introduction . . . . . . . .‘. . 4.2. The Cooccurrence Restrictions of COOCCURRENCE Prestem O O O O Q Morphemes. . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2.1. The Subject Marker . . . . . 4.2.2. The Infinitive Morpheme. . . . 4.2.3. The Tense Morphemes. . . . . 4.2.4. The Subject Relative Morpheme. 4.2.5. The Conditional Morpheme . . . 4.2.6. The Focus Morpheme . . . . . . 4.2.7. The Negative Morphemes . . . . 4.3. The Cooccurrence Restrictions of Poststem Morphemes . . . . . . . 4.3.1. The Indicative Morpheme. . . . 4.3.2. The Subjunctive Morpheme . . . 4.3.3. The Tense Suffix ~11g. . . . . 4.3.4. The Locative, Manner, Intensifier and Question Morphemes . . . .'. . . 4.3.4.1. Additional Restrictions on the Question Morphemes . . . . . . . 4.3.4.2. Additional Restrictions on the Manner Morpheme. . . . . . . . . 4 Morphemes. . . . . . . . . . . . 4.4.1. The Object Marker. . . . . . . 4.4.2. The Reflexive Marker . . . . . 4.4.3. The Verbal Extensions. . . . . 4.4.3.1. The Causative. . . . . . . . 4.4.3.2. The Applied. . . . . . . . . 4.4.3.3. The Passive. . . . . . . . . ix .4 The Cooccurrence Restrictions of Stem Page 103 104 107 109 109 113 113 114 115 115 125 126 127 129 132 134 135 138 139 140 152 152 154 154 156 162 164 167 170 173 176 176 177 178 180 180 181 182 182 185 188 188 190 193 mmmmmmmm hOONt-a hhhtfihhhfi 1 2 3 4.7. and Non—Stem Morphemes . . . Cooccurrence Restrictions and Verb Types 4. The Recriprocal. .5. The Repetitive . . . . . . . . . . . .6. The Stative. . . . . . . . . . . . . Cooccurrence Restrictions between Stem The Subjective . . . . . . . The Subject Marker . . . . . . . . The Question Marker. . . . The Manner Marker. . . . . Intransitive Verbs . . . . . . . . . Fossilized Intransitive Verbs. . . . . . Transitive Verbs . . . . . . . . . . . Summary and Conclusion . . . . . . . . . Chapter 4: Endnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER 5: 5.1. 5.2. 5.2.1 GOOQNNNNNN OIUIUIOIUIOIOIUICHOIUTUIO'I NH Ocmcnowmcnowmcn wcnoswcnoaucn :. . . 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 t ap CHAPTER 6: CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS 6.1. 6.2. Suggestions for further research Summary. . . . . . . . . . 6.3. Contribution . . . . . . . PRINCIPLES OF VERBAL MORPHOLOGY . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . B's theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Predictions arising from Relevance and Generality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .1. Relevance. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .1.1. Order. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .1.2. Frequency. . . . . . . . . . . . . .1.3. Degree of fusion . . . . . . . . . .2. Generality . . . . . . . . . . . . . .2.1. Order. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .2.2. Frequency. . . . . . . . . . . . . .2.3. Degree of fusion . . . . . . . . . . Synthesis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comments on B’s theory . . . . . . . . Haya as a target of study. . . . . . . . . Typological position of Haya . . . . . . Appropriateness of Haya as a test of 8’s theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Application of the theory to Haya. . . .1. The Prestem Morphemes (Non-Stem) . . .2. The Stem Morphemes . . . . . . . . . .3. The Poststem Morphemes (non-stem). . .4. The Subject Marker and Object Marker .5. The Split between sets of morphems . .6. Castairs theory. . . . . . . . . . . .7. Summary and conclusion . . . . . . . er 5: Endnotes . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 194 197 197 199 199 200 201 201 202 203 205 208 209 213 216 216 216 219 221 221 223 224 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 235 239 242 242 244 245 246 267 274 275 277 281 281 283 283 Page APPENDICES I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 285 APPENDIX A I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 285 APPENDIX B. . . 299 BIBLIOGRAPHY I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 306 xi Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table 10 12 13 Distinctive Features of Consonant and Glides. . Distinctive Features with Redundancy Consonant Phonemes Rules: Distinctive Features of Vowel Phonemes. Distinctive Features with Redundancy Vowel Phonemes Rules: Distribution of Consonants before Vowels. . The Internal Structure of Conjugated LIST OF TABLES Verb in Haya. Inventory of Morphemes and Allomorphs List of Noun Classes and Subject Morpheme Markers. . Haya Affirmative and Negative Tenses. Subject Relative Morphemes. Direct/Indirect Objects . . . . . . . Summary of Cooccurrence Restrictions of Prestem Morphemes. Morphemes xii 'Cooccurrence Restrictions of Stem Page 17 19 21 22 32 146 147 148 149 150 151 214 215 Alv. Alv. Pal. ant bk C cf 00118 cont 001‘ DA DE fn FRP GC GEN hi IF IFH INFL IP In Vb LIST OF SYMBOLS AND ABBREVIATIONS Alveolar Alveolar Palatal Anterior Back Consonant Confer ’compare’ Consonant(al) Continuant Corona Doer of Action Distinctive Features Foot Note Far Remote Past Glide Consonant Generality Human High Immediate Future Immediate Future Habitual Inflectional Immediate Past Inflected Verb .xiii IVb Lab. Dent. lo MHV nas NC NH NF NPH NPS NP(s) NPSu NPt OSuV OVSu PAT pl(p) PUH R1--R13 RLV RMF RMP RP Sg(s) son SuOV Instransitive Verb. Labio-Dental Low Morphemic Formula of the Haya Verb Nasal Nasal Consonant Non-Human Near Future Near Past Habitual Near Past Noun Phrase(s) Noun Phrase Subject Noun Phrase Topic Object Subject Verb Object Verb Subject Patient Plural Present Universal Habitual Rules (1-13) Relevance Remote Future Remote Past Recent Past Singular Sonorant Subject Object Verb xiv SuVo' STL Str Syl TOP UNR Vcd VP VOSu VSuO WLT Subject Verb Object Still (tense) Strudent Syllabic Topic Unreal (immaginary) Verb Voiced Verb Phrase Verb Object Subject Verb Subject Object Weak Lexalist Hypothesis XV CHAPTER 1 BACKGROUND INFORMATION 1.1. Goals In this dissertation we discuss the linear order and co-occurrence restrictions of the Haya verb morphemes. The theoretical basis employed in this study is that of Bybee (henceforth B) (1985), which is discussed in chapter 5. This study has three related goats. The first is to identify the Haya verb morphemes and their linear ordering and to answer the question "is the linear ordering of the Haya verb morphemes predictable?" This study will attempt to make generalizations that capture the predictions of linear ordering. If the answer to the question above is yes, which our data seem to suggest, then the second goal is to find out the factors which determine this ordering. In this study we will argues that the linear ordering of the Haya verb morphemes is determined by phonological, syntactic, semantic and pragmatic factors. The third goal which is closely related to the above, is to determine whether these factors follow B’s theory, if not, we shall seek how B’s theory can be modified to incorporate these factors? Finally, this 2 work, as far as we'know, will be the first detailed study of Haya verb morphoplogy. The comparison of B’s work and the present study reveal both significant similarities and major differences, a point we will further explore in. detail in chapter 5. Our procedure is as follows: a. outline B’s main hypothesis and her arguments. b. show the possibility of reinterpreting B’s work. 0. present the Haya data, and how it could be interpreted with regard to B’s claims. d. Finally, give the discrepancies between the data and B’s claims we propose a framework in which to view the data. In the course of our discussion we show how some of B’s arguments are supported by our data. This leads us to our major goal of exploring and developing a theoretical framework in terms of which the linear ordering of the Haya verb morphemes can be understood and in terms of which the Haya verb structure in general can be described. That there is usually a fixed linear order of morphemes particularly in agglutinating languages, is not an issues, but rather how to comprehend, them. There are several proposals, both specific and general which state how such a linear order can be determined. In view of the limitations of these proposals, particularly with regard to Haya this dissertation presents another such proposal, one that differs from previous proposals mainly in that it attempts to be highly restrictive, and, in particular, it deals specifically with the Haya language. Unlike> many’ previous studies of languages including that. of B, that have been based on a wide variety of languages (50 languages in B’s study), our work focuses on a single language. As further argued in chapter 5, this approach has an important advantage of depth over a comparative approach, as Chomsky (1985:6) argues: "A valid observation that has frequently been made (and often, irrationally denied) is that a great deal can be learned about Universal Grammar from a study of a singlewlanguage, if such study achieves sufficient depth to put forth rules or principles that have explanatory force. . . ." (emphasis mine). Our study undertakes an in-depth study of Haya verb structure in order to uncover sufficient detail about the nature of Haya linear ordering so that we can offer an evaluation, in light of B’s work. 1.2. The Language Haya is a Bantu language spoken in the Northwestern part of Tanzania to the west of Lake Victoria. It is spoken in the Bukoba and Muleba districts in the Kagera region. 1.2.1. Classification Guthrie (1970:13) classified Haya under zone E.20 Haya— Jita group which includes: E.21 Nyambo (Karagwe) E.22 Ziba (erroneously called Haya) E.23 Dzindza (Jinja) E.24 Kerebe (ukerewe) E.25 Jita (Kwaya) However, scholars including Copes (1971), Meeussen (1976), and Nurse (1969) seem to disagree with him in his separation of some languages in zones D and E. The more convincing classification of the Haya language is by Meeussen (1976) whose classification is composed of Guthrie’s zone D and E. This classification is composed of the languages most chsely related to Haya: Meeussen’s classification Guthrie’s Zone Country Nyankore E:13 Uganda Ciga E:14 Uganda Ganda E:15 Uganda Nyoro E:11 Uganda Nyaruanda 'D:61 Ruanda Rundi D:62 Burundi Subi D:64 - Tanzania Ha D:66 Tanzania Vinza D:67 Tanzania The classification issue is not settled yet. In a recent study, Nurse (1979:2) quoted Mosha as saying that 5 "The Ugandan survey indicated that several of the 'interlacustrine languages (which included. Haya) form. one large group, which is called Rutara, . . . . The languages involved. were .Nyankore, Kiga, Nyoro, and. Tooro."l Nurse further quoting Mosha further notes that "From the Tanzanian point of 'view, Rutara should be expanded to include Haya (with. ‘Nyambo), Zinza, and. IKerewe." Regarding the relationship between these languages he points out that, "All these languages are almost equally close: a speaker of any of them listening to another on the radio would have little brouble understanding; they all operate variants of the same tone system. . . .” Nurse (1979:2) also notes that "Rutara is also linked to: a. Ha, Hangaza, Shubi, Ruanda, Rundi, Vinza; b. Jita, Kwaya, Ruri, Regi; c. Gusii, Kuria, Ngurumi, etc; d. Luhya; e. Ganda, Soga, Gwere, etc. . . . and these together form what is called the Lacustrine group."2 1.2.2. Dialects of Haya Haya has eight dialects, corresponding to the location of former chiefdoms: Dialect Chiefdom Ziba Kiziba Hamba Kianja Hangiro Ihangiro Nyakisaka Misenye Yoza Kyamtwara Endangabo Bugabo Bumbira Bukara Mwani Karagwe The dialects are mutually intelligible. A speaker of any of these dialects has little trouble understanding any of the others, as the author can testify such as he is a native speaker of the Ziba dialect. 1.3. The People The speakers of the language known as AbaHaya, ’the Haya people’ call their language OluHaya. The Haya speakers mainly inhabit Bukoba and Muleba districts in' the Kagera region in Tanzania.3 They are surrounded by speakers of other Bantu languages: Nyaruanda (Ruanda) to the west, Ciga, Ganda (Uganda) in the north; Zinza and Hangaza (Tanzania) to the south. On the eastern side is lake Victoria. However, Hayas are currently scattered all over Tanzania mainly in 7 the cities of Dar-es-Salaam and Mwanza working in government and private offices, and some engaging in commerce. The population of the Haya people is estimated at about a million or more. This is merely an estimate because the 1977 Tanzanian census abolished the identification of the population by their ethnic groups.‘ Haya living in the rural areas are primarily farmers; they grow bananas and plantains which constitute their staple food. Beans, sweet potatoes, yams, millet, corn, tomatoes and cassava are supplements. of their diet. The principle cash crops are coffee and tea. Other Haya people who live near lake Victoria and along the banks of river Kagera do some fishing in addition to farming. The Haya also raise cattle, goats and other livestock. They also engage in small commercial activities. 1.4. Publications and data 1.4.1. Published material As far as literature is concerned, there are few published materials on the Haya language in general and no extensive work on the morphology of the Haya verb. General works on Haya include that of Betbeder and Jones (1949); Rascher (1958) on the grammar of the Haya language; Bona- Baisi (1950), a Haya dictionary; Byarushengo (1975), a phonological sketech of Haya; Byarushengo et al. (1977) Haya grammatical structure; Hyman and Byarushengo, a model 8 of Haya verb tonology in Clements and Goldsmith (1985).5 Other related works on Haya include: Rubanza (1979), the relationship between Kiswahili and other African langauges: the case of Haya and Seitel (1980) a collection of Haya stories, proverbs and heroic poetry. There is also literature on the history of Haya such as that of Lwamgira’s (1949) "History of Kim," one of the former chiefdom of Hayaland, and various Bible, song and religious primers. In addition, there exists a monthly' newspaper, RUMULI ’the torch,’ published by the Bukoba Roman Catholic Diocese. Some of the data of this dissertation comes from the above citations and specifically from the following articles: Byarushengo (1977:205-213), a word list; Dalgish (1977) ’Past tense formation in OruHaya’ and Seitel’s Haya epic collection (1970’s-1’980’s).6 The author of this dissertation, a native speaker of the language, also provides a major source of the data. 1.5. Organisation of the dissertation In chapter 2 we will outline a phonological sketch of Haya. Particular attention is paid to the aspects of phonology that are of relevance to the analysis of the Haya verb in general and the linear order of the Haya verb morphemes in particular. The phonological outline is provided to assist the reader and preclude discussion every time phonology comes up in the course of the morphological analysis. Moreover, the provision of the phonological 9 outline allows the handling of a particular alternation as morphophonemic if and when it ‘cannot be explained phonologically. These reasons provide the justification for having a chapter on phonology when, as a matter of fact, the present work deals mainly with morphology, a point further taken up in chapter 2. Chapter 3 contain an inventory and provides a detailed description of the morphemes that constitute the Haya verb. The functions of each of these morphemes is given. The order of the morphemes is not discussed a task which is the basis of chapter 4. Chapter 4 specifically deals with the linear order of the verbal morphemes and their co-occurrence restrictions. In this chapter two claims are made and substantiated: a. that there is a interrelationship between the location of the stem morphemes and the root, depending on the number of arguments a verb can take. b. that four related factors determine the order of occurrence and co-occurrence; restrictions of the Haya verb morphemes: (i) a phonological factor, (ii) a syntactic factor, (iii) a semantic factor, and (iv) a pragmatic factor. Chapter 5 lays out the essential points of B’s theory. Her two major principles Relevance and Generality are discussed and exemplified especially with respect to the predictions they make concerning ordering, frequency and 10 degree of fusion. Chapter 5 also discusses the implications of the Haya data to B’s theory. This exercise sorts out those claims that are supported by the Haya data and those that are not. In the light of these developments, we offer modified version of B’s theory regarding linear ordering. Chapter 6 summarizes of the main findings of the dissertation, suggestions for future research. These suggestions are drawn from the findings of the Haya data, which have relevance to other Bantu languages in particular and other languages of the world in general. 11 CHAPTER 1 ENDNOTES 1 Nurse (1979:146) says in a footnote that Mosha’s remarks are from Ladefoged (1972:69). 3 Nurse (1979:2) claims that he used what he termed a Lexico-statistical method which is not defined at least in this paper. 3 Bukoba and Muleba have been two separate districts since the mid 1970’s. Otherwise it was one district (Bukoba). 4 The current population data on Tanzania is that of 1977, in which the population of the residence of Bukoba and Karagwe districts were. Karagwe 185,016 Bukoba rural 256,349 Muleba 217,493 Bukoba urban 77,519 738,277 Rate of growth from the 1977 census was + 42.5%. But as it was mentioned elsewhere, Hayas are scattered all over the region. and the» nation, particularly' in big cities (see Bureau of Statistics (1982)). 5 Hyman and Byarushengo’s (1984) work is the only detailed study on Haya tone we know of. The work is enriched with many examples at the end of the chapter. 5 I am deeply grateful to Dr. Peter Seitel who allowed me to use part of the data he collected from Bukoba. (see appendix 1). CHAPTER 2 PHONOLOGICAL SKETCH OF HAYA 2.1. Introduction The presence of a chapter on phonology in the present study of morphology is explained in this introductory section.1 In addition to accurately representing distinctions in information needed to understand the surface morphological. representations a jphonological. study should reveal an interrelationships between the study of phonology and the study of morphology, as well as other parts of grammar. This is especially true of morphology as Selkirk (1982:59) who, when dealing with affixation argues that "Any theory of affixation must allow for grammars that represent explicitly and perspicuously the grammatically relevant information that is idiosyncratically associated with a particular affix morpheme." Selkirk further notes that this information is of three varieties namely: syntactic, semantic and phonological. Regarding the phonological variety, Selkirk (ibidz63) notes that the first property to be represented is information concerning pronunciation of the affix. "This will include; minimally a distinctive feature matrix representing the composition of the affix." Finally, Selkirk mentions what he called the idiosyncratic 12 13 phonological property of affixes, which is "their phonologically unpredictable effect on the pronunciation of surrounding morphemes, for example, certain affixes trigger certain types of allomorphy in other morphemes" (Cf Dell and Selkirk (1978), Aronoff (1976)). In the current study, the description of phonological processes has been limited to those that have impact on the Haya verb morphology. The basic features of phonemes of the language are first given followed by redundancy and phonological rules. 2.2. Phonological Inventory Haya phonemes are consonants (including true consonants and glides) and vowels distributed segmentally and sequentially in the syllable. Vowel phonemes are syllabic and tone bearing and obligatory in every word. Haya has: 19 consonant phonemes 10 vowel phonemes 2 tones. 2.2.1. Inventory of Consonant Phonemes. There are nineteen consonant phonemes in Haya, given in (1) and Illustrated in (2):2 l4 (1) Bilabial Lab.Dent Alv. Alv.pal. Pal. Velar Glottal Stops p,b t,d k.g Affricate 5,! Fricative f s,z s h Nasal m n ‘3 Lateral l Glide . y w The alveopalatal phonemes will henceforth be orthographically written as follows: /é/ = ch /5/ = J /§/ = sh /K/ = ny The phoneme 1 has two allophones: [l] and [r]. In this work the same symbol 1 is used for both [1] and [r]. All of the above phonemes have the phonetic value usually associated with these phonemic representations, except for /l/, which has free variants [l] and [r]. ~(2) /P/ /t/ juguta ’blow’ paikuka ’to be uncontrolled’ tiila ’borrow’ e.g. child’ paapala empako /b/ bona shoba gamba lk/ lekela ziika kuula lg/ gaga yoga ngoma /f/ enfuka faakala 15 ’to get lost e.g. in unknown land’ ’a cave’ ’see’ ’30 bad’ ’say’ ’cease’ ’bury’ ’uproot’ ’go bad’ ’take a shower’ ’drum’ ’a hole’ ’make a mistake (sin)’ fafalikana ’become unhappy’ kuntila ’foot fall’ /d/ tonda ’create’ dindind ’deep inside’ dini ’religion’ /é/ chumba ’cook’ chuchula ’comb hair’ okuchwa ’to spit’ /5/ juba ’fish’ olubaju ’side’ janjaba ’help e.g. a sick person’ /8/ sinda ’moan’ musigazi ’a youth’ lemesa ’cause someone to be tired’ /z/ zimba ’swell’ gendeleza ’continue, develop’ nzilo ’dirt’ lé/ shashula mushanju gasha lm/ manya omwongu lemala /3/ nyiikila enyenje okumanya ly/ yanika iya hooya 2I2I1I1I The distinctive features for these consonant phonemes are given in table 1.3 and [-consonantal]. (see 2.2.2.1). 16 ’pay back’ ’seven’ ’be useful’ ’know’ ’pumpkins’ ’become lame’ ’put more effort’ ’mane’ ’to know’ ’put on the sun to dry’ ’uproot’ ’sing’ [+consonantal]. The Vowels are glides /h/ hanuula bihuka omuguha /n/ inuka igana nigaala /l/ laalika shaalila ilagula /W/ wetole iwe [+syllabic] are and ’give advise’ ’insects’ ’r0pe’ ’go home from work’ ’be equal’ ’get anger’ ’invite’ ’become bitter’ ’become black’ ’steal’ ryour Distinctive Features of Consonants Phonemes All true consonants are {-syllabic] l-syllabic] {-consonantal] 17 Table 1. Distinctive Features of Consonant and Glides. syl. SOU- ‘ - - - - - - - - - - - - + + + + + + cons.+ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + - - cont.- - - - - - - - + + + + + - - - - + + nas. - - - - - - — — - - — — — + + + - - - ant. + + + + - - - - + + + '- - + + _ + - _ cor. - - + + + + - - - + + + - - + - + - - vcd. - + - + - + - + - - + - - + + + + + + str - - - - + + - - - + + + — - - - - - - 2.2.1.2. Redundancy Rules for Consonant Phonemes Important redundancy rules pertaining to consonants and glides are: (3) R1 F . -syl --> -nas -cons ~ant +cont +son . J R2 +cons +cont --> [-vcd] -cor R3 [+son] --> [+vcd] R4 R5 R7 R8 R9 R10 18 ‘80!) [+cons] —-> [-syl] [-son] --> {-nas} [+nas] -—> -cont. +son -cor --> [-str] +ant -cont -son --> {-cor} -cont -ant -son --> [+str] +cont +cor Table 2 is a repeat of table accounted for by redundancy rules in 1 10 refer to redundancy rules. 1 (3). with some spaces The numbers e.g. 19 Table 2. Distinctive Features with Redundancy Rules: Consonant Phonemes. pbtdéjkgtszéhmnxlyw syl 4 4 4 4 4 son - - - - - cons + + + + + cont - - - - - nas 5 5 5 5 5 ant + + + + - cor - - + + + vcd - + - + - str 8 8 - - + - - — + + + + - 7 7 7 1 1 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 + + + - 1 1 - - - + + + - - + + - + 1 1 + 9 9 - + + + - - + + + + - + - + 2 10 10 10 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 2.2.2. Haya has ten phonemic vowels, (of. illustrated in (5). (4) i ii also Byarushengo Inventory of Vowel Phonemes five short and five long 1977: 3-4) as given in (4) and u uu o 00 a aa These have the phonetic values usually associated with their phonemic representations. 20 (5) i sima ’knock ii siima ’thank something’ (someone)’ i bika ’announce ii biika ’keep’ death’ e tega ’set up a ee teega ’curse’ trap’ I lega ’trap water’ leega ’strech’ u balula ’burst, open’ uu baluula ’tell’ chuchula ’comb hair’ chuuchuula ’run fast’ 0 kolola ’cough’ oo kooloola ’bring things improperly’ yola ’get poor’ yoola ’get things from the ground’ a laza ’set fire wood aa laaza ’extinguish’ _ extensively’ shalila ’take off leaves shaalila ’become bitter’ of the banana plant’ 2.2.2.1. Distinctive Features of Vowel Phonemes The Haya vowels are categorized in a distinctive feature) (DF) matrix in table 3. All vowels share the feature [+syllabic] which distinguishes vowels from consonants. Phonetically, short and long vowels occur, but phonologically the long vowels will be analyzed as sequences of two vowels. 21 (7) Va Va --> lV:] Table 3. Distinctive Features of Vowel Phonemes. 1 e a o u syllabic + + + + + high + - - - + low - — + - - back - - + + + 2.2.2.2. Redundancy Rules for Vowel Phonemes. Certain redundancy rules allow us to predict some features in the DF matrix. Certain feature values are abstracted from table 3 by redundancy rules R11-R13 (3) R 11 [+hi] --> {-101 R 12 [+10] --> [-hi, +bk] R 13 [-bk] --> {-10} Table 4 include numbers that correspond to the redundancy rules in (8) above. 22 Table 4. Distinctive Features with Redundancy Rules: Vowel Phonemes. i e a o u syllabic + + + + + high + - 12 - + low 11 13 + - 11 back - - 12 + + 2.3. The Tones Two levels of tones are distinguished: High (H) and Low (L) as exemplified below:‘ Note that debate of whether or not tone is a feature of vowel or syllable is scope of this study: (9) ’pregnancy’ ’louse’ ’break’ ’her husband’ ’steal’ ’finish’ ’intestines’ ’pull’ ’inherent’ beyond the 23 The two functions of tone are of two types: (1) Lexical (2) Grammatical 2.3.1. Lexical Tone Lexical tone in Haya as in other Bantu language serves as a feature distinguishing one lexical item from another as shown in (9). 2.3.2. Grammatical Tone Grammatical tone in Haya distinguishes number, and tense/aspect. For example a high tone pattern in the present universal habitual tense, distinguishes the first person singular from the third person singular: (10) n -5 -gend-‘ SM-TNS-go -IND ’I am going’ n -a -gé'nd-A’? SM-TNS-go -IND ’is he going?’ In what follows, tone will not be marked unless it distinguishes lexical and/or grammatical categories. 24 2.4. Distribution of Consonant and Vowel Phonemes. The distribution and frequency of occurrence of consonant and vowel phonemes in Haya words are discussed and exemplified below. 2.4.1. Distribution of Consonants 2.4.1.1 Distribution of Consonants Occurring Singly All consonants phonemes occur singly word initially, and between vowels.5 One example of each occurrence is given for each consonant phoneme. See more examples in appendix B. (11) Initial Position /p/ pima ’measure’ /b/ byaala ’plant’ /t/ telela ’slip’ /d/ dura ’to uplift-oneself’ /k/ kinga ’close e.g. door’ /g/ gangabula ’split’ /5/ chunda ’shake’ /3/' juna ’help’ /f/ fuha ’blow nose’ /s/ sindika ’ ’push’ /z/ zana ’play’ /§/ shona ’sew’ /h/ hooya ’sing’ /m/ /n/ NH /1/ /y/ /w/ (12) Between /p/ /b/ /t/ /k/ /3/ /5/ /3’/ /f/ /s/ /z/ /§/ /h/ /m/ /n/ /?f/ /1/ HI AM 25 myama nunka nyyama lobeka yoleka wazambuka Vowels paapala babiilila yetonge ikala bugangana chachalika yajula fufuza sisiimuka bazangana shashula hohahoba lemala nanura monyoka guluka gayangana wawalikana ’sleep’ ’smell’ ’sleep’ ’soak’ ’show’ ’come out of sleep suddenly’ ’to run out of fear’ ’itch’ ’deny’ ’dwell’ ’meet’ ’talk nonsense’ ’crow’ ’to go in a wrong direction’ 'wake up’ ’ask each other’ ’pay back’ ’to be frightened’ ’become lame’ ’cure’ ’break off’ ’jump’ ’despice each other’ ’to be unclear of what one is doing’ 26 The phoneme (1 does not appear between. vowels (see 2.5.1.2.1). There are no word final consonants. 2.4.1.2. Consonant Clusters There are two types of consonant clusters: (a) a nasal consonant followed by another consonant (henceforth abbreviated as NC), and (b) a glide followed by another (true) consonant (henceforth GC). /Np/ /Nb/ /Nt/ /Nd/ /Nk/ /N8/ /N6/ /N3/ /Nf/ /N8/ /N2/ /N§/ -mpako -mbuzi -ntale -ndwana -nkilabinu -nkeremeke -ngoma -nchumita -njoka -mfuzi -nseiso -nzilo -nshanani ’cave’ ’goat’ ’lion’ ’1 fight’ ’the middle finger’ ’baby’ ’drum’ ’1 spear’ ’snake’ ’orphan’ ’a stone used to grind’ ’dirt’ ’gap (tooth)’ 27 (13.b) NC: between vowels /Np/ -mpolampola ’slowly’ /Nb/ -kambula ’tear down’ /Nt/ -muntu ’a person’ /Nd/ -muhanda ’path' /Nk/ -mukankaburo ’a banana which is not mature’ /Ng/ -omungemu ’in the banana field’ /N6/ -okuchinja ’to slaughter /N3/ -mwekubanjuro(s) ’tiredness’ /Nf/ -enfuka ’a hole’ /Ns/ -sinsimuko ’dirt’ /Nz/ -enkwanzi ’bead’ The following examples show the GC consonant clusters in medial position which is the only position in which they occur. Although. they' are [geyja] ’become fat’ boigola --> [boygola] ’bark’ chaichiliza --> [6ayéiliza] ’tease’ 28 2.4.2. Distribution of Vowels 2.4.2.1. Short vowels Short vowel phonemes appear word initially, between consonants and word finally as in (15)-(17). (15) Initial /i/ ikiliza ’agree’ inama ’bend’ ija ’come’ /e/ eya ’sweep’ enda ’break’ ebembela ’lead e.g. way’ /u/ ulila ’listen’ umuza ’put down’ uma ’become blind’ /o/ oloola ’answer’ ondela ’follow’ oba ’be frightened’ /a/ ababikana ’come out with a torrent of words’ abya ’demolish e.g. a house’ ahula ’clean the house’ In dialects of Haya other than the Ziba dialect the vowel phoneme u does not occur in word initial position, instead we find hm in those dialects. (16) a (17) Between Consonants (18) Final /i/ /e/ /u/ /o/ la/ /i/ bika hika lila leka lela tema juma kula bula bona yoga shomba baza shaba kama mulimi muzaani mwombeki 29 ’announce death’ ’arrive’ ’cry’ ’leave (it)’ ’bring up child’ ’cut down’ ’abuse’ ’grow’ ’get lost’ ’see’ ’bathe’ ’carry things’ ’ask’ ’as for’ ’milk cow’ ’farmer’ ’player’ ’builder’ 30 /e/ a-lagil—e ’he should give message’ SM-give-SUBJ ba-kul-e ’they should grow’ SM—grow-SUBJ tu-jun-e ’We should help’ SM-help—SUBJ /u/ ichumu ’spear’ ndimu ’lime’ bizimu ’ghosts’ /o/ koyo ’a small heart’ kalimo ’a small job’ mulalilo ’promise’ /a/ lagila ’give message’ kula ’grow up (in age)’ kulima ’farming’ The short vowels i, _u_, g, and a appear word finally in nouns, a appearing only in infinitival nouns. Verbs always end in a, and only end in g in subjunctive and perfective forms. 2.4.2.2. Long Vowels Long vowel phonemes appear between true consonants and glides: 31 (19) /i/ biiya ’become bad’ ziika ’bury’ biika ’keep’ O/e/ leeba ’look’ teega ’curse’ teema ’play around’ /u/ juula ’undress’ baluula ’tell’ shuula ’greet’ /o/ goola ’ashame’ yoola ’get things from the ground’ shooba ’go slowly’ /a/ zaana ’play’ shaaga ’remainfl shaabuka ’cross over’ Long vowels appear neither word-initially nor word finally. 2.4.3. Distribution of Consonants Before Vowels. Table 5 shows the distribution of consonant phonemes before vowel phonemes word initially. It shows that the consonants j, g and the semi-vowels x and w are not followed by the vowel i. The vowel e does not follow the consonants n and d. The consonant phoneme d is also not followed by the phoneme vowel a. Consonants s, g and the semi-vowel g are not followed by the vowel u. These cases are regarded as accidental gaps in the language (except w before u which 32 Table 5. Distribution of Consonants before Vowels.6 /i/ /e/ /a/ /o/ /u/ /p/ + - + + + /b/ + + + + + /t/ + + + + + /d/ + - - + + /k/ + + + + + /g/ + + + + + /e/ + + + + + /j/ - + + + + /f/ + + + + + /s/ + + + + - /z/ + + + + - /§/ - + + + + /h/ + + + + + /m/ + - + ' + + /n/ ~ + + + + + /3/ . I + + + + + /1/ + + + + + /y/ 1 - + ‘ + + + /w/ - . + + - - 33 is principled). This needs historical explanation, which is beyond this study. 2.5. Phonological Rules The following phonological rules affecting vowels and consonants, are observed in Haya: Consonants: Nasal assimilation and consonant alternation. Vowels: gliding, vowel deletion, and vowel lengthening. 2.5.1 Phonological Rules Affecting Consonants The following are phonological rules affecting consonants: 2.5.1.1. Nasal Assimilation A nasal shares place of articulation with the following consonant. (20) B cor [+ nas] --> [a ant] / ---- [- syl] 34 (21) n-bul -a --> m-bula --> mbula I-get lost-IND ’I get lost’ n-b -a -—> m-ba --> mba I-be-IND ’I become’ n-f —a --> m-fa --> mfa I-die-IND 'I die’ The data show that the phoneme n always assimilates to the place of articulation of the following consonant within a word" The n- above represents the first person subject marker in a verb. As mentioned in section 2.3.2. this rule only concerns the only prefix n of the language (the first person). In addition, the nasal becomes syllabic word initially if followed by a non-nasal consonant. (22) [+nas] --> [+syl]/# --- -syl -nas (23) n-pa --> [mpa] ’I give' n-bika --> [mbika] ’I announce death’ n-kunga --> [nkunga] ’I shout’ 35 However, if the nasal consonant is followed by another nasal consonant, the nasal cluster consonant is shortened. (24) [+nas] [+nas] --> [+nas]/# --- (25) n-manya --> manya ’I know’ n-mila --> mila . ’I swallow’ n-mala --> mala ’I finish’ 2.5.1.2. Consonant Alternation The following alternation occur in Haya.7 2.5.1.2.1. Strengthening The contrast of I and d is neutralized in favor of g, after a nasal g. (26) C +cor +ant --> [-son]/[+nas] --- -nas -cont (27) a. o -n -lim -ila --> ondimila SM-OM-STEM- you-me-farm-for-BEN—IND ’You farm for me’ 36 b. a —n -lum -a --> anduma SM-OM-STEM-IND he-me-bite-IND ’he bites me’ o. n -leet -a --> ndeeta SM-bring-IND ’I bring’ Another example of strengthening is exhibited in the neutralization of h, and p in favor of p after the nasal phoneme n. (28) C -cor -vcd --> [-cont]/[+nas] --- -str ' (29) a. n-hango --> m-pango --> mpango it (e.g. cow) big ’it is big’ b. a -n -hiig -a --> a-m-piiga --> ampiiga SM-OM-STEM -IND he-me-look for-IND ’he looks for me’ 37 c. n -hum -a --> m-puma --> mpuma SM-STEM -IND I -blind—IND ’I become blind’ 2.5.1.2.2. Spirantization The contrast between the three consonants I, d and yg is neutralized in favor of z before the high front vowel i. (30) v +COI‘ +vcd --> [+contl/ ---- +hi -cont -bk (31) a. (i) a -kol -a SM-work-IND a -kol -ile --> akozile SM-work-TNS ’he/she has worked’ (ii) ba-laa-shol -a SM-TNS-pay tax-IND ba -laa-shol -ile --> balashozile they-TNS-pay tax-TNS ’they have already paid tax’ 38 b.(i) mu-lond-a SM-pick-IND mu -lond-ile --> mulonzile you(pl)-pick-TNS ’you(pl) picked’ (ii) ba-lind -a SM-guard-IND ba-lind-i --> balinzi SM-guard ’guards’ c.(i) ba-enj -a SM-search-IND ba-enj-i --> baenzi ’searchers’ (ii) mu-ganj -a SM-be loved-IND mu-ganj—i --> muganzi ’lover’ Finally, the contrast of sh and s is neutralized in favor of s before i. ~39 (32) r “ V -son +cont --> [+ant]/ -—-- +hi +cor -bk -vcd (33) a. shash-a STEM —IND ’get sick’ shash -il -a --> shasila get sick-APL-IND ’get sick/hurt for . . .’ b. lash -a throw-IND lash -il -a --> lasila throw-APL-IND ’throw at/for . . .’ See also 2.5.1.2.4 (37) on page 42). 2.5.1.2.3. Palatalization The contrast between the nasal phonemes n neutralized in favor of n before the high vowel i. (34) v +nas --> [-ant]/ ---- +hi +cor -bk and :1 is 40 (35) a. ni -n-0 -ikiliz-a --> ni-ny-ikiliza --> ninyikiliza FOC I-TNS-agree -IND ’I am agreeing’ b. n-O -inam-a --> ny-inama --> nyinama I-TNS-bend-IND ’I bend’ c. ba -n -0 -iluk-a --> ba—ny-iluka --> banyiluka they-me-TNS-run -IND ’they run away from me’ 2.5.1.2.4. Morphophonemic Rules Consonant alternations involving t, g, g, and s are phonologically unnatural, and are morphologically conditioned. (36) t a —--> s/ ---- -ile recent past where -ilg is the suffix recent past and the discontinuous morphemes of the near tense, explained fully in chapter 3 page 76. 41. (37) a.(i) a -it -a SM-kill-IND a -it -ile --> aisile he—kill-TNS ’he has killed’ (ii) mu-jugut-a SM-blow -IND mu -jugut-ile --> mujugusile you(pl)-blow -TNS ’you have blown’ b.(i) n-chaich -a I-disturb-IND (ii) n-chaich -ile --> nchaisile I-disturb-TNS ’I got disturbed’ c.(i) tu-laa-shash -a SM-TNS-get sick-IND "we-will get sick’ (ii) tu-la -shash -ile --> tulashaasile SM-TNS-get sick-INS ’we have already got sick’ As evidence of the above claim, we cite examples in (38) where no alternations of 111.5: 5!; occur in the phonological 42 context —ilg. The s s alternations do appear, (38.0) as we explained in (32). (38) a.(i) ba-it -a SM-kill-IND ’they kill’ (ii) ba -it —il -a --> baitila They kill-APL-IND ’they kill for/at . . . ’ b.(i) n-chaich -a I disturb-IND ’I disturb’ (ii) n-chaich ~iliz-a -—> nchaichiliza I-disturb-RPT -IND ’I become disturbed repeatedly’ 2.5.2. Phonological Rules Affecting Vowels The phonological rules affecting vowels in Haya are documented in Byarushengo (1975, 1977). A few examples and illustrations are shown here. The rules are: glide formation, vowel deletion and vowel lengthening. 2.5.2.1. Glide Formation The contrasts of i and e with y and of u with 31 are neutralized in favor of glide before vowels. 43 (39) a. V {-bk] --> -syl -—-- V [+hi J b. V [+bk ] --> {-syl] / --- V +hi (40) a. i. ku —li -a --> kulya INF-eat-IND ’to eat/eating’ ii. ba-laa-li-end -a —-> balyenda SM-TNS-OM like-IND ’They will like it’ iii. ba-li -ki-ongel-a --> bakyongela SM-TNS-OM-add IND ’they will add to it’ iv. tu-O -bi-umuz -a --> tubyumuza SM-TNS-OM-put down-IND ’we put them down’ As was pointed out in rule 16, in most of the other Haya dialcts, the last example would be realized as tu-bi-umuza —-> tubihumuza ’we put them down,’ because the phoneme u is realized as hu verb root initially. 44 (40) b. i. ku -igan -a -—> kwigana INF-be equal-IND ’to be equal’ 11. ku -ey -a --> kweya INF-sweep-IND ’to sweep’ iii. ku -og -a --> kwoga INF-swim-IND ’to swim’ iv. ku -at -a --> kwata INF-break-IND ’to break, to catch’ Rule (7) repeated here as (41), which forms a long vowel from a phonological cluster of like vowels applies before (39) to prevent 11 --> 2;, up --> HQ etc. (41) V¢V¢ --> [V:] 2.5.2.2. Vowel Deletion In Haya, there are exceptions to rule (39.a.b.). Byarushengo (1977:5) correctly indicates that although both the negative marker ti- and the pmesent universal habitual’ marker ni- end in i, this vowel does not undergo glide formation. Instead, it undergoes vowel deletion. However, this process is morphologically conditioned, appearing only after negative (NEG) and the present universal habitual. 45 (42) v NEG; --> g / ---- - hi FOC + bk (43) a. ti -a -gend-a --> tagenda NEG SM-go -IND ’he doesn’t go’ b. ni -a -gend-a --> nagenda FOC-SM-go -IND ’he is going’ c. ni -o -shom -a --> noshoma FOC-SM-study-IND ’you are studying’ 2.5.2.3. Vowel Lengthening Vowels are predictably long in Haya as in Kinyaruanda, a language closely related to Haya (Kimenyi (1979)) in the following environments: (1) when a morpheme ending in a vowel precedes another morpheme that begins with an identical vowel, (2) when a.vowel precedes a nasal cluster within stems, and (3) before the morpheme n - ’I/me.’ 46 2.5.2.3.1. Lengthening of Identical Vowel Sequences When a morpheme ending in a vowel precedes another morpheme that begins with an identical vowel, the sequence of the two identical vowels are realized as long as was shown in rule (7) repeated here as (44). (44) v.v._--> [v:] (45) a. ku -li -ik-a --> kuliika INF-eat-st-IND ’to be edible’ b. ku -ulil -a --> kuulila INF-listen-IND ’to listen’ 0. ba-a -lim —a --> baalima SM-TNS-cultivate-IND ’they are cultivating’ 2.5.2.3.2. Lengthening before Nasal Clusters Vowels are lengthened if they occur before nasal consonant clusters. 47 (46) linda --> liznda ’wait’ tunga --> tuznga ’get rich’ kenga --> keznga ’suspect’ mu ’ -shondo --> musho:ndo ’gift’ SM(-¢)-8ift lamba --> lazmba ’lick’ (47)a. [+ syl J [+syl] --> + long / ----- NC, where NC is nasal + cosonant clusteru But because we .have regarded the .DF [+long] as redundant in Haya, (47.a) can be formulated as (47.b). b. V --> V:/ ---- NC 2.4.2.3.3. Lengthering before n A ‘vowel is lengthened if it occurs preceeding the morpheme n- ’me’ (48) a. ku -n -mil -a --> ku:mila INF-OM-swallow IND ’to swallow me’ 48 b. ku -n -mal -a --> ku:mala INF-OM-finish-IND ’to finish me’ c. ku -n -nag -a --> ku:naga INF-OM-throw-IND ’to throw me’ (not take care of me) Lengthening can also be interpreted as in (49). (49) + syl [+syll] --> + long / --- [+nas] [+nas] Rule (49), however, is just a special case of (48), with the simultaneous nasal assimilation (rule (20)), and the nasal cluster shortening (rule (24)). 2.6. A Word on Syllable Structure Haya has two types of syllable structures: Closed and open. The closed syllables, are of two kinds: (a) those ending in glides (see also glide formation page 42) and (b) those ending in nasals (see consonant clusters, section 2.4.1.2). Examples of closed sylables ending in glides are in (14) repeated here as (50). 49 (50) geija --> [geyja] ’become fat’ boigola --> [boygola] ’bark’ chaichiliza --> [éayéiliza] ’tease’ Examples of closed syllables ending in nasals are in (13) repeated here as (51). (51) kambula ’tear down’ sinsimuko ’dirt’ mpolampola ’slowly’ Otherwise Haya has open syllables. The syllable final vowels are either short or long. An abstract notion of the syllable structure, containing one obligatory vowel is exemplified in (52), in which G represents glides and N represents nasals and C represents all consonants. (52) (N)(C)(G)V(=) The representation in (52) is partially illustrated in (53) immediately followed by two examples for each representation. 5O (52) (N)(C)(G)V(2) (a) NCGV: (b) NOV: (0) NV: (d) NV (e) CGV: (f) CGV (8) GV: (h) GV (i) V: (J) V (53) a. NCGV: o -mu-mbyaazi --> omumbyazzi IV-SM-planting field ’in the planting field’ b. NGV: a ~myaam-e -—> amxaame SM-sleep-SUBJ ’he should sleep’ 0. NV: amaani --> ama:ni ’strength’ d. NV tu-mal -a --> tumala SM-finish-IND 'we finish’ e. CGV: f. CGV g. GV h. GV 1. V: j. V 51 kxaala --> kyazla ’take a walk’ luala --> lwala ’get sick’ ba-yggI-a --> bayoola SM-pick-IND ’they pick things from the ground’ mu-ta -xasham -a --> mutayashama SM-NEGz open mouth-IND ’you should not open your mouth’ ta -leeb-a --> taleeba NEG-look-IND ’he/she does not look’ ichumu ’spear’ 2.7. General Remarks and Summary In this chapter selected. aspects of phonology applicable to the understanding of the morphology of the Haya word have been discussed and exemplified, namely: the phonological inventory, (including distinctive features and 52 redundancy rules), the distribution of Haya phonemes, certain phonological and morphophonemic rules, and, briefly Haya syllable structure. These various aspects of phonology will be assumed in later chapters which deal with descriptive and theoretical issues of Haya verb morphology. 53 CHAPTER 2 ENDNOTES 1 For a detailed sketch of Haya phonology, see Byarushengo (1975). 3 There are few lexical items in Haya beginning with the phonemes ./p/, /d/ and /w/. There are for instance 30 entries of #P - in the Haya dictionary, Bona-Baisi (1957), 7 entries of #d-, and 14 entries of #w-. 3 We have selected features that capture the necessary distinctions in this language. Some features are acoustic while others are articulatory. (Jakobson et al. 1951, Jakobson and Hale 1956, Chomsky and Halle 1968). Note that there is not DF specification for phonemes /m/and/n/on syl because they could either be syllabic or non-syllabic (see for example rule (22) page 34). ‘ Haya appears to be among Bantu Languages which have undergone partial reduction. in tonal distinctiveness and tonal morphologization. 5 In the examples given a verb is regarded as the simple stem and an indicative marker. Where no such examples exist a noun has been given. ' In the rest of this work tables will be placed at the end of each chapter. 7 For a detailed analysis of consonant alternations, see Lee Trithart in Byarushengo et al. (1977). CHAPTER 3 THE MORPHEMES OF THE VERB: FORMS AND FUNCTIONS 3.1. Introduction This chapter describes the morphemes of the Haya verb in terms of their meaning and their linear order within the verb. The cooccurrence restrictions which hold among these morphemes are the topic of chapter 4. The twenty three verbal morphemes to be described are listed in table 6 at the end of this chapter followed by an inventory of all morphemes with their allomorphs in table 7. We represent the twenty three morphemes in. a morphemic formula in (1) (henceforth PHD”. .As will be seen in this chapter, the Haya Verbal morphemes may be divided into two groups: (a) The Root, and (b) The Non-Stem Morphemes. 54 .«toaaago c. cummsom_u ma conga ox“. golgs manna .ao.mo_o=o=a =-LLuo u.“ «.93. Laooc-oo mocogaco. .clzv u>.xs_.o. «:2 can .zm. casca- sooaasm as. cogs .o .o._a a La .aoaaa >~¢ mu: was >_=o .m .uaa .o usulxa. a mo~_a.~ Ea..om.oa=m a .o mocomaa o=_ .. “MW .Amm..a : amo- Ea L=ooo >a. x "ex ..o... .Loa.== oz. .o ..-._ as. o. a: do moo:o..=ooo o.a...=. a mean. .~=_. ..oa. .n .zma. Luxcac .ooansm o>~za.oa a cue. m._=mo. gm uca >_ .c =o_8~=.a.oo o=_ .N .amco, 00., .0: .sn an .o oo_ogo as» mo.a-mmooo= >_ as «usage «4. .— .musoz 2-3-3-2- 2::- :- 2-25-3-3: : s 5...; m A A - o - m $-13: com a :2: as 3.. :E-a-Eaa- 2% l a..- :c- «31:22 12.7521 . 5.. . I:£51.:rag-32:;€5-22-2.8 a2. mm¢a mace >— ..>==. m¢u> ¢»c= ux_ me «Jagged e.guzamcz ._. 56 3.2. The Root We may begin the description of the verbal morphemes in Haya with a description of the root, which may be considered to be the minimal lexical unit of the verb. In this study, Driever’s (1976: 23) definition of root in Swahili, another Bantu language, may be adopted for Haya: "the part of the verbal expression which carries the lexical meaning of the verb and cannot be morphologically analyzed into smaller meaningful units." IExamples of such. minimal meaningful units are given as follows: Iroot ’die’ b. -lum- root ’bite’ root ’fury’ d. -zal- root ’give birth’ 57 The presence of hyphens on either side of' root forms indicates the absence of any other morphological material. A verb root may coocur with a large number of verbal morphemes, whose description is the purpose of this chapter. First, a root may cooccur with morphemes which modify its lexical meaning or alter its argument structure: the verbal extensions.2 One example, which will be discussed in more detail later, is an extension which, among other functions adds a beneficiary argument:3 (3) -zal-il- ROOTrAPL- ’give birth for’ Second, a root may occur with morphemes which encode direct (DO) and indirect objects (IO): the valence morphemes.‘ The previous example may take a morpheme encoding the beneficiary argument: (4) a. mu-zal -il- DO-give birth-for ’give birth for him’ b. mu-ba-jun -il- ° DO-IO-help for ’help him for them’ 58 At this point we define another term: the stem. The term stem will be used to refer to the set of all monomorphemic roots plus the set of all roots with verbal extensions and/or valence morphemes.5 It should be noted however that no other source (”1 any other Bantu language includes the valence morphemes as part of the stem. Third, a root may cooccur with morphemes indicating tense/aspect, mood, finiteness, negation, conditionality, focus, status as a relative clause and agreement for person, gender and number of the subject of the verb. One example, whose details are again to be discussed later, may be given as follows: (5) a. a -ka -zal- 3Psg-TNS-give birth ’she gave birth’ where 3Psg is the third person singular (future representation will be modified), and TNS refers to tense to the specifics of which will be discussed later. b. 0 -li -bon- 2Psg-TNS-see ’you(sg) will see.’ Three examples of apparently maximal verbs generated by the morphemic formula of the Haya verb (MHV) are: (6) a. ka -ba —ta -li-ku -mu-tel -el -a -ha -ge -kwo. COND-SM -NEG2-NF-INF-OM-ROOT-APL-IND-LOC-MNR-INT if -they-not- -ing-him-hit well-indeed ’If they are not indeed hitting him well here’ b. ti -tu-ka -ba -lim -is -e -ki- NEG1-SM-RMP-OM -ROOT-CAUS-SUBJ Q Not-we- -them-farm- - -what- ’What haven’t we caused them to farm?’ 0. ka -mu -ta -ba -mu-leb -angan-il -a -ha -ge -kwo 2 -5 -3 -9a -9b-10 -16 -13 -18 -20 -21 -23 COND-SM-NEG -DO -IO-ROOT-RCP -APL-IND LOC-MNR-INT. ’If you are not indeed taking care of him well for one another here.’ From these examples it becomes apparent that it is impossible to get all the 23 morpheme in one verbal cOnstruction. There are certain co-occurrence restrictions on a number of them the details of which are discussed in chapter 4. We will later present criteria to show that these two groups of morphemes, the morphemes constituting the stem as the first group and all the other morphemes as the second group, may be reasonably regarded as constituting separate sets. 60 The remainder of the chapter is organized as follows. The second group of morphemes (the non-stem morphemes) are presented first. They are described in relation to the stem, which will be referred to simply as "the stem" without reference to its internal structure. Each morpheme will be presented as part of one or more verb forms. Next, the stem morphemes are presented” The reason for this order of presentation is that the stem morphemes are mostly morphemes which affect the argument structure of roots. For this reason, they are best illustrated with sentences rather than isolated verb forms, as in the case of the non-stem morphemes. Prior description of the non-stem morphemes will make these examples easier to understand. 3.3. The Non-Stem Morphemes 3.3.1. The Indicative Morpheme We may begin the description of the verbal morphemes of Haya with the indicative morpheme :a, sometimes called the imperative. This morpheme occurs directly following the verb stem, schematized as follows: (7) STEM - IND The indicative morpheme in combination with the verb stem forms an imperative: 61 (8) a. abazaile ba-laa-gul-a ebitabo parents SM -IF -buy—IND books The parents will buy the books. b. iba ya-leet —a ebilaatwa husband-SM-bring-IND shoes ’her husband brought shoes’ c. nyowe nyw -a amaizi gangi bulikilo I drink-IND water plenty every day ’I drink plenty of water every day.’ The indicative also occurs in other forms of the verb as well, as will be discussed as the dissertation develops. 3.3.2. The Infinitive Morpheme The infinitive morpheme kg; realized as kg and kg (see chapter 2 page 44), is attached to the stem, with final vowel :a, without tense markers (It may be followed by the (NEGz) if there is one). (9) INF - (NEGz) - STEM - (10) a. ku -shom-a INF-read-IND ’to study/studying" 62 b. ku -ib -a --> kwiba INF-steal-IND ’to steal/stealing’ c. ku -ta -gab -a INF-NEG: give-IND ’not to give/not giving’ The infinitive form of the verb has both nominal and verbal syntactic functions, the description of which is beyond the scope of this study. When the infinitive is nominal, it controls the agreement of class 15. (11) a. O -ku -shom-a ku-tin -is -a abantu IV-INF-read—IND 15-be afraid-CAUS-IND people. ’Reading frightens people’ b. O -ku -lim -a ku-jun -a IV-INF-farm-IND 15-help-IND ’farming helps’ The SM(INF) agrees with the nominal infinitive which in the examples above is preceded by an initial vowel (IV). We will at a later stage discuss and exemplify the noun classes in Haya in section 3.3.3.1. 63 3.3.3. The Subject Marker The subject marker is a verbal morpheme which shows agreement with the subject of the verb. It may be exemplified with the present universal/habitual tense (see section (3.3.5.1.1.)), of the verb, in which it is prefixed to the verb stem fellowed by the indicative morpheme: (12) a. ba-lim -a SM-farm-IND ’they farm (usually)’ b. a -guz -a SM-sell-IND ’he sells (usually)’ The precise linear order of the subject marker with respect to other pre-stem verbal morphemes will become clear below when these morphemes are discussed. 3.3.3.1. The Noun-Class System More specifically, the subject marker agrees in noun- class with the subject of the verb. In order to understand the degree or variation found in the subject marker, we will briefly discuss the noun-class system in Haya. Nouns in Haya, as in many other Bantu languages, belong to concordial classes, which are referred to as noun- classes. Nouns which belong to the same noun-class control 64 the same set of agreement affixes on verbs and modifiers (cf. Bennett 1982). In addition, the nouns belonging to a single noun-class tend to be marked with morphologically identical noun-class prefix, although this generalization is not absolute. There are eighteen noun-classes in Haya. In this dissertation, we will use the numbering system for the noun-class system of Bantu languages referred to as Welmers system (1973). Under this numbering system, the first ten noun classes form singular/plural pairs, as shown in table 8 (page 148). Odd-numbered classes have their plurals in the following even-numbered. classes. 131 addition, Class 11 nouns have their plurals in Class 10.‘ Class 12 (ka-) is predominantly the class of diminutives (Ashton et al. 1954 in Luganda). The prefix ka- which usually follows prefixed to the initial vowel a- (aka-), may replace any noun in order to indicate the diminutive form sometimes slightly changing the original meaning as exemplified below: (13) a. O -mu-ntu a -ka-ntu IV-lZ-STEM— IV-lZ-STEM ’a person’ ' ’a small thing’ b. O -mu-ti a -ka-ti IV-3 -STEM IV-12-STEM ’a tree’ ’a small stick’ 65 c. e -li-sho a -ka-isho IV-5 -STEM IV-lZ-STEM ’an eye’ ’a small eye’ whereas example (13.0) shows the diminutive form of an eye, in (13.a) the meaning attached to the diminutive form is not of a person but of a ’small’ thing. Class 13 (tu-) may function as a plural form of class 12. (14) a. a —ka-ntu o -tu-ntu IV-lZ-STEM IV-13-STEM ’a small thing’ ’small things’ b. a -ka-isho o -tu-isho IV-lZ-STEM IV-13-STEM ’a small eye’ ’small eyes’ Class 14 (bu-) is a class of the augmentative of the plural diminutive class 13 and may as well function as the plural augmentative form of class 12 (ka-). (15) a. a -ka-ntu o -bu-ntu IV-12-STEM IV-14-STEM ’a small thing’ ’little bigger things’ 66 b. o -tu-ntu o —bu-ntu IV-13-STEM IV-14-STEM ’small many things’ ’little bigger many things’ Class 15 consists entirely of infinitive forms and some body parts: (16) a. o -ku-andika IV-15—STEM ’writing/to write’ b. o -ku-gulu IV-15-STEM ’a leg’ when class 15 is used as a body part, it takes the plural form of class 6 (ma-). (17) o -ku-gulu a -ma-gulu IV-lS-STEM IV-6 -STEM ’a leg’ ’legs’ Noun classes 16-18 are traditionally referred to as locative classes (Rascher ibid.: 65-69).7 These classes are indicated by the morpheme marker ha-(16), ku-(17) and mu- (18) with the underlying idea of definite place, indefinite 67 place and. withinness (inside) respectivelyu These noun classes will become more clearer in the course of the discussion. In classes 1 and 2, the classes of singular and plural human beings, respectively, the verbal subject markers vary for lst, 2nd and 3rd person as well as for class, as seen in table 8 and exemplified in (18) and (19). (18) a. n -chumb-a buli kilo ls-cook -IND every day ’I cook every day’ b. 0 -lim -a ekilo Zs-farm-IND night ’you(sg) farm at night’ 0. a -yombek-e amaju 3s-build -SUBJ houses ’he should build houses’ (19) a. tu-guz -a emotoka lp-sell-IND cars ’we sell cars’ b. mu-bug -a emotoka eziango ' 2p-drive-IND cars big ’you drive big cars’ 68 c. ba-s -e omugusha 3p-grind—SUBJ millet o’they should grind the millet’ Since nearly every Haya verb example in this dissertation contains a subject marker, they will not be exemplified further here. 3.3.4. The Subjunctive Morpheme The subjunctive form of the verb is formed with the subject marker prefixed directly to the verb stem, which is followed by the vowel :e in place of the indicative vowel :a, schematized in (20) and exemplified in (21):° (20) SM - STEM - SUBJ (21) a. ni -bend-a ba-lim -g ekibanja FOC-STEM-IND 2 -farm-SUBJ garden ’they want to farm the garden' b. a—gend-e Bukoba? I-STEM-SUBJ ’should he go to Bukoba?’ 69 c. ba-shom-e ebalua ba-many-e agalimu 2 -STEM-SUBJ letter 2 -STEM-SUBJ content ’they should read the letter in order to know the content’ d. a-tel -g . ente ezi? I-STEM-SUBJ cows these ’should he hit these cows?’ The subjunctive morpheme is used in different ways including but not limited to: following verb like want (21.a), when permission is sought (21.b), when the intention is to express purpose (21.0). Other functions not exemplified above include: serving as a more polite imperative, used in indirect commands; with the question morpheme :ki; it questions the possibility. With the locative morpheme it questions the location. Both of these possibilities are exemplified below: (22) a. ba-lim -e -ki 2 -farm-SUBJ—Q ’what should they farm?’ b. a-shuntam-e -ha I-sit -SUBJ-LOC ’where should he sit’ 70 The question and locative morphemes will be discussed in detail in section 3.3.10. 3.3.5. The Tenses As far as the present author can determine, there are twelve tenses in Haya (see also section 3.3.8), which are listed in table 9 (page 146).9 It is not the purpose of this work to provide an exhaustive description of the semantics and usage of each tense. Such a description is probably not possible for the tense system of any Bantu language at the current state of our knowledge. The following descriptions of tenses in Haya are intended to be working definitions only, to facilitate an understanding of tense morphology. They' will be based on ‘broad conceptions of ’a present moment,’ ’time before the present time’ and 'time after the present moment’ developed in Moshi (1988) (for Chaga, a Bantu language related to Haya.) Within these three categories, there are further distinctions made, which will be brought up in the course of the discussion of each tense. It will also be apparent that several of the tenses involve elements of meaning which are more properly described as aspectual, but while we will note such elements, we have not attempted to systematize them, again because they fall beyond the scope of this work, which is intended to focus on the organization of verbal morphology. 71 3.3.5.1. The Present Tense Moshi’s conception of present time probably is used in referring to situations which occupy a much longer period of time than the present moment, that is, those which include the present moment (see Comrie, 1985: 37).10 If such a conception is true, then Moshi’s concept of the present tense fits into the description of the Haya present tense. Present’ tense in Haya includes the actions, states and processes which hold at the present moment, but which began before the present moment and which may well continue for a short period of time after the present moment as exemplified below: (23) a. Omwalimu mukulu n -a -nyegez -a abagenyi teacher old Is-IR-welcome-IND visitors ’the principal is inviting visitors’ b. Abajaji ni -ba-lamul -a omushango guango judges FOC 2 judging-IND case big ’the judges are judging the big case.’ In each of these examples, it is true that the actions of welcoming, and judging hold’ at the present moment. However, these actions may have started two or more hours before and may continue for some time. Therefore it is accurate to say that the basic meaning of the present tense in Haya as in many other languages, locates a situation at 72 the present moment, but the situation may continue beyond the present moment, or may also hold in the past-~depending on the event. We may now discuss the formation of the present tenses in Haya beginning with the present universal habitual (PUH). 3.3.5.1.1. Present Universal Habitual The present universal habitual tense is formed by attaching the subject marker directly to the verb stem, which is followed by the indicative morpheme, schematized as follows: SM - STEM - IND This tense refers to habitual, ongoing, repetitive actions, as seen in the following examples: (24) a. ba-lim -a 3 -farm-IND ’they farm’ b. mu-it -a 2p-kill-IND ’You(pl) kill’ 73 3.3.5.1.2. "Still" Tense The "still" tense is formed by attaching the morpheme :kza; directly preceding the verb stem. The morpheme :kya; is itself directly preceded by the subject marker, and the indicative morpheme is suffixed to the verb stem, schematized as follows: SM - STL - STEM - IND The "still" tense is a present tense which also carries the more aspectual implication that the action or state is still in progress. It is illustrated in the following examples: (25) a. ba-kya-lim —a 2 -STL-cultivate-IND ’they are still cultivating’ b. zi-kya-nyw -a 10-STL-drink-IND ’they(e.g. cow) are still drinking’ In example (25.b) the class 108M refers to ente ’cows,’ while in (25.0) the class 148M refers to obunaku ’poverty’ 0. bu-kya-ij -a 14-STL-come-IND ’poverty is still coming’ 74 3.3.5.2. The Past Tenses The past tenses are used to describe a situation holding sometime before the present moment. Moshi (ibid:2) mentions that in Kivunjo the past represents a three way distinction: P1(-a-) within few minutes, P2(-am-) within few hours and P3(-le-) furthest point in the past. In Haya there are separate forms of the verb referring to six periods of the past time, the immediate past (to-day), the near past (very early to-day) the recent past (yesterday), the remote past (day before yesterday), and the far remote past (long ago). These will be described in more detail in the following sections. In addition, the near past has a habitual form. These tenses are listed in table 9 on page 147. 3.3.5.2.1. Immediate Past. The immediate past tense is formed by attaching the immediate past tense morpheme :a; (IP) directly preceding the verb stem. This tense morpheme is in turn directly. preceded by the subject marker. The indicative morpheme is suffixed to the verb stem. The formation of the immediate past is therefore parallel to the formation of the "still" tense, and all tense morphemes to be described in the chapter will be seen to occupy the same prestem position except for the near past (NPS), the far remote past (FRP), and the near past habitual (NPH) all of which, in addition, 75 have morphemes suffixed to the verb-stem. The recent past (yesterday) only has the morpheme following the stem. This tense may be schematized as in (24): (24) SM - IP - STEM - IND This tense is used to refer to an event or state which occurred or was in force at some very immediately previous point on the day of speaking and is now finished. We may now’ propose that this tense refers to events that have occurred within the past few hours (three to four hours). It may not refer to a time other than to-day. (27) a. gu-a -gw —a 3 -IP¥fall-IND ’it (tree) fell (to-day)’ b. tu-a -guz —a Ip-IP-sell-IND ’we sold (to-day)’ 3.3.5.2.2. Near Past .The near past is marked by prefixing the immediate tense morpheme :_a__-_ (NPS) to the verb stem and by suffixing the stem with the morpheme :ilg (which replaces the indicative morpheme).11 This tense may be schematized as: 76 ooooo The near past tense is used to refer to an event or state which occurred or was in force at some point early on the day of speaking. The event might have happened five to six hours previously, such that this tense is usually used to demarcate the day into morning, afternoon, or evening. Examples of the recent past tense are given below: (29) a. tu-a-gul-ilg (--> twaguzile) ebitooke bwanka 1p- -buy- bananas morning ’we bought bananas this morning’ b. kya—g-endek-ilg nyemishana kyashubamu 7- -break- afternoon repeat kuendeka mbwenu aha to break to-day now ’It (the chair) broke this afternoon and now it has repeated breaking again’ 3.3.5.2.3. Recent Past The recent past is marked only by the suffix _;_i_l_g, attached directly‘to the verb stem, in the place of the indicative morpheme._ The verb stem is directly preceded by the subject marker, and there is no present tense Omarker parallel to the tense marker :3; of the near past. The recent past may be schematized as follows: 77 (30) SM - STEM - RP This tense is used to refer to an event or state which occurred or was in force at some point the day before the day of speaking and is now finished. Examples of the recent past tenses are given in (31). (31) a. a-lim -ile I-farm-RP ’he/she farmed (yesterday)’ b. li-endek-ile 5 -break-RP ’it (the tooth) broke’ The suggested referent in (31.b) (the tooth) in the gloss belong to noun class 5 (see noun classes 144). The recent pastin Haya is cognate to a Kamba recent or previous day's past tense (Farnsworth 1952:26) where the tense morpheme is pa with -ig suffixed to the verb-stem. Probably the gig now-0‘...- 3.3.5.2.4. Remote Past The remote past has a morphological structure which is parallel to the near past rather than the recent past. There is a tense marker :ka; (RMT) which is prefixed 78 directly to the verb stem. and preceded by the subject marker. The indicative morpheme is suffixed to the stem. The remote past may be schematized as follows: (32) SM - RMT - STEM - IND This tense is used to refer to an event or state which occurred or was in force at some point before on the day before, usually one year or more, and is now finished. It is not easy to delineate the exact time in the past to which this tense refers, nor is it possible to state how long ago the action took place or the state was in force. The reference made above is only a guiding definition of this tense. The remote past tense in Haya seems to be the same as that in Runyankore, another Bantu language closely related to Haya (Morris and Kirwan 1957). As in Haya, to form this tense in Runyankore -ka- is inserted between the subject prefix and the stem. Examples of the remote past tense in Haya are: (33) a. n -ka -tel-a Is-RMT-hit-IND ’I hit (last week)’ b. tu-ka -bandam -a Ip-RMT-crounch-IND ’we crounched (e.g. last year)’ 79 3.3..5.2.5. Far Remote Past The far remote past is also a combination of a prefixal tense morpheme and a suffixal one. As in the near past tense, the suffixal morpheme is :ile. attached directly to the verb stem. The prefix, again preceded by the subject marker and attached directly to the verb stem, is the morpheme :_I_a;. The far remote past may be schematized as follows: (34) SM - Ia - STEM - lie This tense is used to refer to an event or state which occurred or was in force at some point a long time previously, usually more than two years. Again this time is not an absolute but simply a relative time. Nurse (1979:12) calls this tense the"have already’ (long ago), while both Rasher (ibid) and Byarunshengo et al. (ibid) fail to make a note of this tense. This tense is exemplified as follows: (35) a. e-la-SW-ilfi 4 fell ’they (trees) have already fell (a long time ago)’ where the referent trees belong to noun class 4. 80 ‘b. ba-Ia-yombek-ilg 2 build ’they have already build (e.g. three years ago)’ 3.3.5.2.6. Immediate Past Habitual The formation of the immediate past habitual is nearly identical to that of the immediate past. The tense morpheme :a; (IPH) directly precedes the verb stem and is in turn directly preceded by the subject. marker. The indicative morpheme is suffixed to the verb stem. Unlike the present universal habitual, the immediate past habitual contains a morpheme specifically indicating habitual action: the habitual morpheme _-__a_g_-_, which is suffixed directly to the verb stem preceding the indicative morpheme.12 The immediate past habitual may be schematized as follows: (36) SM - a - STEM - ag - IND The immediate past habitual indicates actions or situations that hold at all times, or repetitive actions or events.13 The immediate past habitual differs from the immediate past in the’time slot. Whereas the immediate past refers to events that have taken place on the day of speaking, the. immediate past habitual includes both the actions of the immediate past and near past which occur repeatedly. In other contextual situations, the immediate past habitual may 81 also includes the concept of the possibility of not repeating the action again as shown in examples below: (37) tu-a-lim-ag—a (bilebi tu-ka-Iekel—a) Ip- -farm- IND these days SM- stop IND ’we were habitually farming (we have in these days stopped)’ Other examples of the near past habitual are: (38) a. ba-a-beiy-ag-a 2- -lie- -IND ’they were usually lying’ b. n —a—yeseS-§£‘a Is- -teach- -IND ’I was usually teaching’ 3.3.5.3. The Future Tenses The future tenses are used to describe an event or state which will occur or hold sometime subsequent to the present moment. In other words, the most typical uses of the future tense involve events or states that are planned to happen or to be implemented sometime subsequent to the present time. However, there are certain future categories which do not involve the prediction of an action or state if 82 there is no belief or intention of implementing these actions as exemplified below: (39) n-laa-tem -a ente (ndaatema) Is-IF-slaughter-IND cow ’1 will slaughter the cow’ The prediction cu? ’slaughtering the cow’ cannot be implemented if the speaker believes that the slaughtering will actually not take place. Again as in earlier definitions of tenses, the definition of the future tense is given as a guideline, but the actual meaning is context as well as intentional sensitive. As for the phonological changes shown in example (39), see chapter 2 section (2.5.1). Haya has separate forms of the verb referring to three periods of future time parallel to those we have seen for the past tenses: the immediate future, the near future and the remote future, which. will. be described. in. more detail in the following sections. In addition, as in the past tense series, the near future has a habitual form. These tenses are listed in table 9 on page (147). 3.3.5.3.1. Immediate Future The three future tenses (apart from the habitual) are formed exclusively with prefixal morphemes parallel to those we saw in the formation of the past and perfect tenses. There is no suffixal morpheme involved in the formation of 83 the future tenses. The immediate future (IF) is formed by attaching the morpheme :laa; directly to the verb stem, in the same position relative to the stem as the past and perfect prefixal morphemes, preceded, as before, by the subject marker. The indicative marker is suffixed to the stem. The immediate future may be schematized as follows: (40) SM - Ian - STEM - IND This tense is used to refer to an event or state which will take place or be in force sometime today or tomorrow. Again the immediate future tense is relative, ”where the reference point for location of a situation is some point in time given by the context, not necessarily the present moment" (Comrie ibid:56). Comrie’s explanation is made clear by the following Haya examples: (41) Yesu a-laa-lind -a abakristu bona Jesus I-IF -protect-IND Christians all aba-laa-ondel -a obugolozi bwe who-IF -follow-IND leadership his ’Jesus will protect all those who will follow his leadership’ The interpretation which can be given to example (41) is that the present moment is relative to the time when the statement was uttered. For someone who heard it ten years 84 ago this statement is not taking place on the day it was spoken (for the first time) or on the day after the day of speaking but it has been taking place for ten years prior to ten years ago and will take place after the present moment for an indefinite time (as long as he/she is alive). Compare example (41) to (42). (42) ba-laa-gend-a Tanzania omwezi ogulaija 2 -IF -go -IND month next ’they will go to Tanzania next month’ In this example, the month in which this utterance is made is taken as a ’reference point to-day.’ If it is uttered in July, then their going to Tanzania is in August, regardless of when in August. Other examples which exhibit the use of the immediate future tenses in Haya are: (43) a. ga-laa-yatik-a 10-IF -break-IND ’they (eggs) will break’ (to-day or to-morrow). Here again the suggested referents ’eggs’ belong to noun class 10. b. tu-laa-gul-a ebitabo Ip-IF -buy-IND books ’we will buy (to-morrow) books’ 3.3.5.3.2. Near Future The near future (NF) is formed by prefixing the morpheme 3,1,1": directly to the verb stem, preceded by the subject marker. The indicative marker is suffixed to the stem. The near future may be schematized as follows: (44) SM - I; - STEM - IND This tense is used to refer to an event or state which will take place or be in force the day after tomorrow. Again the near future tense is relative depending on context as illustrated in the following Haya examples. (45) a. Omwana ogu a-li-bug -a endege child this-I-NF-drive-IND planes ’this child will be a pilot (lit:driver of planes)’ The time reference to this sentence, although it is basically sometime in the future, is not clear, because it depends on the age of the child and the laws of countries for one to become a pilot. If the child is four years old and the minimum permit age for a pilot is 25 years, then the so called ’near future’ tense is 21 years from the time of the utterance of this statement. Compare example (45.a) to (45.b): 86 (45) b. a-li-zal -il-a omuhosbitali ya Sparrow I-NF-give birth-at-IND hospital of Sparrow ’She will give birth at Sparrow hospital’ If this sentence is uttered to-day and the person talked about is due in two weeks, then the ’near future’ period from to—day as a reference point is two weeks. Thus, again ’the near future tense’ is only realized contextually. Other examples of the near future tense in Haya are as follows: (46) a. enjula e-li-lekel-a kugwa kemulitema emiti rain 4-NF-stop -IND to rain when you cut trees yona all ’the rain will stop raining when you will cut down all the trees’ b. Omuleke, a-li-shwel-a olwo a-li-yend-a leave him alone, I-NF-marry-IND when I-NF-like-IND ’leave him alone he will get married when he will like’H 3.3.5.3.3. Remote Future The remote future (RMF) is formed by prefixing -lika- directly to the verb stem, preceded by the subject marker. 87 The indicative marker is suffixed to the stem. The remote future may be schematized as follows: (47) SM - RMF - STEM - IND The remote future is used to refer to an event or state which will take place or be in force at a point in time farther in the future than that referred to by the near future. We will not discuss the relativeness of this tense as we did in other tenses, except to point out that as the previous tenses, this tense too depends on the context in which it is uttered. Like other future tenses however, it too refers to future events or states without an absolute time reference. Examples of the remote future are as follows: (48) a. O -lika-jum -a Is-RF -abuse-IND ’You will (in the remote future) abuse’ b. zi-lika-ij -a 4 -RF -come-IND ’they (e.g. Monkeys) will (in the remote future) come’ The remote future marker is the only bisyllabic prestem tense marker in the past, present and future series of 88 tenses in the Haya verbal system. This observation raises the question of whether the remote future marker constitutes a unitary morpheme, or whether it is composed of smaller morphemes. It is suggestive that the first syllable of this marker, :1;;, matches the near future marker :11;, segmentally, and the second syllable, :kagJ matches the remote past marker ;k_a_;. It is possible that the remote future marker is composed of the near future marker followed by the remote past. marker, with the semantic effect of indicating a future event or state which also holds at the farthest point in the future. 3.3.5.3.4. Immediate Future Habitual The formation of the near future habitual is patterned on that of the immediate future in the same way that the formation of the near past habitual is patterned on that of the immediate past. In the immediate future habitual, the tense morpheme zlaa; (IFH) directly precedes the verb stem and is in turn directly preceded by the subject marker. The indicative morpheme is suffixed to the verb stem. As in the immediate past habitual, the habitual morpheme gag; is suffixed directly to the verb stem preceding the indicative morpheme. The immediate future habitual may be schematized as follows: (49) SM - Laa - STEM - ag - IND 89 This tense may be generally described as being used for all actions or states which are to take place repeatedly the next. day and also for' any future action for which no definite time is specified (see immediate future 3.3.5.3.1). It differs from the immediate future only in frequency. Whereas actions may take place in the immediate future only once» and. for all, in the immediate future habitual the actions may happen at all future times or be repeated actions. This tense also differs from the present universal habitual in the sense that in the Present habitual the actions are currently taking place repeatedly whereas in the immediate future habitual' the actions are to take place habitually in the future time reference relatively speaking. This tense is illustrated in the following examples: ‘(50) a. tu-Iaa-gul-ag-a Ip- -buy— -IND ’We will (habitually) be buying (e.g. in the evenings)’ b. ba-laa-shom -ag-a 2- -study- -IND ’they will (habitually) be studying (e.g. every night)’ 90 3.3.5.4. The "Imaginary" -ku; tense p I..- an The "imaginary" :ku; (UNR) tense is a conditional tense formed by inserting the morpheme :ku; directly preceding the verb stem in either the immediate past or the remote past, one of the two subject marker. (Note that it may not appear in. the near past :Qggp) . The term "imaginary" 353;, is borrowed from Salone (1977). The "imaginary” :ku; tense may be schematized as follows: (51) SM - g - kg - STEM - IND ._-...J.'._l.e The ”imaginary" 3,1531; tense may be exemplified as follows (note that the tense occurs in both clauses of the conditional): (52) a. ka -n -a -ku -gul-a e—ndege na-a -ku -gy-a COND-Is-IP-UNR—buy-IND IV-plane Is-IP-UNR-go-IND Bulaya Europe ’If I buy a plane, I would go to Europe" b. ka -n -a -ku -mu-bon-ile n -a-ku -mu-siim-ile COND-Is- ~UNR-3s-see -Is- —UNR-Ss-thank- (boine) ’If I would have seen him, I would have thanked him’ 91 It also seems likely that the :ku: of this form is to be identified with the infinitive Ell: rather than one "imaginary” -ku-, since infinitive km; is not preceded by a subject marker, parallel to the -ku- of this form, and unlike "imaginary" :kuz. However, it is also possible that Haya has three distinct kg; morphemes. (We will leave the matter open here. The ”immaginary" :ku; tense is the last tense to. be discussed in this work-15 The discussion of tenses discussed thus far are summarized in table 9 (147). 3.3.6. The Subject relative morpheme The subject relative morpheme is a morpheme which transforms a non-relative verb into a relative verb. Only verbs which have a prestem tense marker are relativized. The subject relative morpheme takes the form of a vowel prefixed to the subject marker. This vowel will be referred to as the initial vowel, following the usage of Ashton et al. for Luganda (1954), Taylor for Nkore-Kiga (1985) languages of Uganda, Morris and Kirwan for Runyankore 1957, and Rascher, 1958 for Haya.1° The subject relative verb may be schematized as follows: (53) IV - SM - TNS - STEM - IND rile IND and _-__i_l._e_ alternate with each other and either is an option in a relative verb as represented above by curly 92 brackets. As its name implies, a subject relative verb is used only when a subject is relativized: (54) a. omuti o -gu-gw -a tree IV-3 -fall-IND ’the tree which fall.’ b. eliino e -li-laa-shash-a tooth IV-5 -IF -hurt -IND ’the tooth which will hurt’ (See table 10 subject relative morphemes page 148). Note that since the head noun of a subject relative is also the subject of the relative verb, the subject marker of the relative verb will agree with it in noun-class.” The identity of the initial vowel is determined by the noun class of the subject marker of the verb: each noun class has its own initial vowel. A list of initial vowels for all of the noun-classes is given in table 10. Note that classes 17 and 18 do not relativize because the relativization made in class 16 covers the other two classes (the locative classes as traditionally known-see fn 7). Although in noun classes 1, 4 and 9, we have included both vowels (the IV and SM), vowel coalescence takes place as was discussed in chapter 2 section 2.5.2. This initial vowel may be compared to the initial vowel of nouns not discussed in this dissertation but may briefly be exemplified here: 93 (55) a. O —mu-ti o —gu-laa-kul -a tu- laa-gu-tem -a IV-3 -STEM IV-3 -IF -STEM-IND 2p(pl) IF -3 -STEM-IND Noun ’The tree (it) which will grow we will cut it’ b. e -mi—ti e -e-laa-kul -a tu-laa-gi-tem -a IV—4 -STEM IV-4-IF -grow-IND Ip-IF -4 -STEM-IND Noun ’the trees (they) which will grow we will cut them’ (56) a. e -ki-tabo e -ki-li-gul—w -a ba-li-ki-shom-a IV-7 -STEM IV—7 ~NF—buy-PASS-IND 2 -NF-7 cread-IND Noun ’the book (it) which will be bought they will read it’ 9b. e -bi-tabo e -bi-li-gul-w -a ba-li-bi-shom-a IV-8 -STEM IV-8 -NF-buy-PASS-IND 2 -NF-8 -read-IND Noun. ’the books (they) which will be bought they will read them’ As exemplified in (55) and (56), the initial vowel in both nouns and verbs omuti/emiti , ’ tree/ trees , ’ ekitabo/ebitabo ’book/books,’ are the same, prefixed to the noun class prefix. However, both the nouns and verbs may not have an 94 IV to which no relativization is realized as in (57) and (58). The passive form (PASS) used in example (54.a) will be discussed in section 3.5.3. (57) a. mu-ti gu-laa-kul-a tu-laa-gu-tem-a ’a tree (it) will grow we will cut it’ b. miti e-laa-kul-a tu-laa-gi-tem-a ’they are trees they will grow we will cut them’ (58) a. kitabo ki-li-gul-w-a ba-li-ki-shom-a ’it is a book it will be bought they will read it’ b. bitabo bi-li—gul-w-a ba-li-bi-shom-a ’they are books they will be bought they will read them.’ The parallelism between the use of IVs with nouns and that with verbs suggests that the addition of the IV to a verb form has the effect of nominalizing it. In support of this suggestion, note that the subject relative verb may function by itself as a nominal, as in the following headless relative construction. 95 (59) a. e -ki-laa-zok -a ki-laa-gul-w -a IV-7 -IF -be found-IND 7 -IF -buy-PASS-IND ’(it) which will be found will be bought ’e.g. a book” 3.3.7. The Conditional Morpheme The conditional morpheme in Haya is the marker kg; which is prefixed directly to the subject marker. When the subject marker has an initial vowel, then coalescence takes place as discussed in chapter 2 section (2.5.2). A verb which contains the conditional morpheme may take a number of meanings, which may be translated using "when," "as," "if" and "let": (60) a. ekitoke ka -ki-kul -a ba-ki-tem-a banana COND-7 -get ripe-IND 2 -7 -cut-IND ’when.the bananas becomes ripe they cut it’ b. ka -ba-laa-b -a ba-laa-ij -a COND-2 -IF -be-IND 2 -IF -COME-IND tu -ba-chumb-il -e Ip(pl)-2 -COOK -APL-SUBJ ’if they are coming let us cook for them’ Note that the APL morpheme -il- which occurs in (60.b) will be discussed fully in section 3.5.2. 96 0. ka -zi-gend-e ku -nyw -a amaizi COND-lO-go -SUBJ INF-drink-IND water ’let them (e.g. cows) go to drink some water’ The conditional morpheme is also used with a special form of the verb, in a construction which Salone (1977:156) calls the "imaginary conditional." Writing on conditions in Haya, Salone argues that there is an imaginary conditional different from other types of conditionals in that the imaginary conditional takes the suffix :ilg as well as the marker :kuz. The suffix :11; as discussed earlier is also used with the near past, recent past, and far remote past. The marker :ku; is placed in the tense marker slot directly preceding the verb stem. Salone calls this marker :53; a marker of "imaginariness" perhaps derived historically from the infinitival marker (see also 3.3.5.4). Its usage is different from its usage as an infinitival marker in several ways. (1) an infinitiver morpheme never occurs with a subject prefix (2) an infinitive morpheme never carries a tense morpheme, both of which do occur with this imaginary indicator: (61) a. n -a -ku -chumb-ile kyonka t -a -li-mu majuta Is-IP-UNR-cook -RP but NEG-IP-be-LOC oil ’I would cook but there is no oil.’ 97 b. ente y-a -ku -za -ile kyonka cow 9-IP-UNR-give birth-RP but y-a -bul -w -a obujuni 9-IP-loose-PASS-IND help ’the cow would have given birth but it lacked help’ 0. abana ka -ba-ku —many-ile ti -ba-ku -las -ile children COND-Z -UNR—know-RP NEG-2 -UNR-throw-RP mabale stones ’if the children would have known, they wouldn’t have throw stones.’ 3.3.8. The Focus Morpheme The focus morpheme in Haya is the marker 31; which is prefixed directly to the subject marker without the initial vowel. We are using the term "focus" to refer to this morpheme because it appears to be cognate to morphemes in other Lacustrine and Thaagicu Bantu languages which have been referred to as a "focus" morphemes, (cf. Clements 1984, Bergvall 1987, Perez (Harford) 1985 for .Kikuyu, Dalgish 1978, Moshi 1986 for Kivunjo, Harford 1987 for Kitharaka; cf. also Bennett 1982 for further discussion and comparison to other Bantu groups). However, the use of this morpheme in Haya is much more restricted than the use of the focus morpheme in these other languages. Moreover, its range of meaning and usage is not ‘well understood in. any Bantu 98 language, although time works just cited represent initial attempts to arrive at such an understanding. In particular, the focus morpheme in Haya is restricted to three verb forms: the present universal habitual, the "still" tense and the Subjunctive: (62) a. Kato na Kwezi ni -ba-lip-a amabanja omuduuka Kato and Kwezi FOO-2 -pay-IND debts store ’Kato and Kwezi are paying debts in the store.’ b. Omwisiki ogu n -a-iluk-a kukila abaisiki bona girl this FOC-I-run -IND more than girls all ’this girl is running faster than all the girls’ 0. Omwishulukale ni -ba-kya-mu-pim -a emyenda the soldier, FOC-2- -OM-measure-IND clothes ’the soldier, they are still measuring him clothes’ In example (62.b) there are phonological changes which take place when the FCC n1; is followed by a vowel subject marker as discussed in chapter 2 section 2.5.2.2. a. abana ni -ba-iluk-e enjula y-a-ij -a children FOC-2 -run -SUBJ rain 9-IP-come-IND ku -gw -a INF-fall-IND ’let the children run it is almost raining’ 99 Example (62.0) show the use of the FOC ni; with the :kya; ’still’ tense. The focus morpheme may be omitted in all of these examples as in (63): (63) a. Kato na Kwezi ba—lip-a amabanja omuduka Kato and Kwezi 2 -pay-IND debts store ’Kato na Kwezi pays debts in the store’ b. Omwisiki ogu a -iluk-a kukila abaisiki bona girl this is-run -IND than girls all ’this girl runs faster than all the girls’ 0. Omwishulukale ba-kya-mu -pim -a emyenda soldier 2 -STL-3s(OM)-measure-IND clothes ’the soldier, they are ’still’ measuring him’ clothes.’ d. abana bai -luk-e enjula yaija kugya children 2-run-SUBJ rain is coming to rain ’the children should run, it is almost raining’ The contrast in meaning between these examples with and without the focus morpheme provides evidence concerning its meaning in Haya. While example (62.a) indicates that the action of paying is still taking place; i.e., a present progressive meaning, example (63.a) indicates that the 100 action of paying debts is habitual, as described above in the section (3.3.5.1.1.) on the present universal habitual. The same meaning contrast may be observed between examples (62.b) and (63.b). Note that our analysis of the role of the focus morpheme differs from that of earlier work on the verbal system of Haya. Previous authors have characterized the present universal habitual with and without the focus morpheme as two separate tenses (Byarushengo 1977, Nurse 1979, Rascher 1958, Betbeder and Jones 1949, Barlow 1951)18 We suggest that there is in fact only one tense, whose meaning may be altered by the addition of this morpheme as described in this sectionl There are two further, observations that may be made in support of this point. First, the focus morpheme may also be attached to nouns in Haya, not just verbs. Although a complete description of this phenomenon is outside of the scope of this work, the following examples may be cited: (64) a. e -ki-shusi ki-a-yat -ik-a IV—7 -guard 7 —break-ST-IND ’the guard broke’ b. n -e -ki-shusi e -ki-a-yat -ik-a FOC-IV-7 -guard IV-7 -break-ST-IND ’it is a guard which broke 101 Example (64.b) indicate that the main function of ni- also in nouns is a "focus marker" as correctly observed by Moshi (1986:2) who stated that Bl: "is relegated to pragmatics by dealing with discourse, intentions and emphasis," (emphasis provided). The use of the focus morpheme with nouns is subject to the same phonological changes noted earlier in its use with verbs. The use of the focus morpheme with both nouns and verbs suggests that it is not a specifically verbal morpheme used in the formation of a single tense. An analysis which posits a separate "present progressive" would be forced to claim that there are two morphemes, one nominal and one verbal. Second, recall that there are other Lacustrine and Thaagicu Bantu languages in which the focus morpheme is used with a much wider range of tenses than in Haya. If the focus morpheme were analyzed as creating new tenses in these languages as well, the number of tenses which they possess would be nearly doubled, and the description of each of these new tenses would be forced to repeat information about the role of focus. Returning to the data, the contrast between examples (62.d) and (63.d) may be described in terms of emphasis: whereas (62.d) is a suggestion, (63.d) is more of a command. Similarly the contrast between (62.0) and (63.0) with and without the focus morpheme may also be described in terms of emphasis: whereas (62.0) may be translated as "the soldier, they are indeed still measuring him clothes", (63.0) is 102 merely stating the facts, that ”they as still measuring the soldier clothes." The meaning of the focus morpheme may also be brought out in more detailed examples which clarify the sorts of contexts in which this morpheme is used. We will cite just one, as follows: (65) "Muliba baberwa, K’abantu balibanoba, ’You will be lucky, when people will avoid you’ ka balibabinga, ka balibajangura, ka ’when they will kick you out, when they will harass you, when’ balibashaima eibala lyanyu okw’obona ’they will throw your name as if’ oti libi, nibabatura Omwana w’omuntu.” ’it is bad, because of a person’s child.’ (Lanctot 1959: 218 omut. Luka 6 (emphasis mine)). From the above description and examples we may generalize that the focus morpheme is restricted; to those verb forms (apart from the infinitive in its verbal uses) which lack a tense marker directly preceding the. stem, apart from the "still" tense, in which the prestem tense marker is arguably aspectual. 103 3.3.9. The Negative Morphemes Haya has three) morphemes indicating negation, which will be referred to as NEGl, NEG2 and NEG3. These three morphemes take different forms , appear in di fferent positions within the verb and are used in different syntactic contexts. 3.3.9.1. NEGl NEGl takes the form ti; (subject to phonological changes; of. chapter 2, section 2.5.2.2) and directly precedes the subject marker. NEGl forms are used exclusively in independent clauses; that is, all clauses which are not relative, conditional, temporal or imperative, the domain of NEGZ (cf. the next section). It may be used with any indicative tense apart from the remote future. NEGl may be exemplified as follows: (66) a. Omushaija ogu a -nyw -a malwa man this Is-drink-IND beer ’this man drinks beer’ b. Omushaija ogu t -a -nyw -a malwa man this NEGl-Is-dink-IND beer ’this man does not dink beer.’ 104 (67) a. Abantu ba Kanyigo ba-laa-yombek-ile eshule people of Kanyigo 2 -IF -build -RP school ’Kanyigo people have already build the school’ b. Abantu ba Kanyigo ti -ba-ka -yombek-ile shule People of Kanyigo NEG1-2 -RMP-build -RP school ’Kanyigo people have not build the school.’ In order to negate the remote future the negative of the near future is used, with an adverbial phrase denoting the appropriate amount of time, as follows: (68) abantu ti -ba-li-lekel-a kuiga maela neila people NEGl-Z -NF-stop -IND to find money forever. ’people will never stop finding money for ever.’ 3.3.9.2. NEGZ NEG2 takes the form ;t§;_ and directly follows the subject marker. As mentioned in the previous section, NEG2 is used to negate verbs occurring in relative, conditional, temporal and imperative clauses. (Cf. Barlow 1951: 88 for the use, in Kikuyu, of a morpheme probably cognate to this one, with a similar if not identical distribution.) Within these limits, it may be used with any verb form, again excepting the remote future (RMF). NEG2 may be exemplified as follows: 105 (69) Relative a. a -ba—laa-shom -a ba-laa-sing-a e -mi-tiani IV-2 -IF -study-IND 2 -IF -pass-IND IV-4 —exam ’those who will study will pass the exams’ b. a -ba-ta -a —shom -e ti -ba-a -sing-e mitiani IV-2 -NEG2-IP-study-SUBJ NEGl-Z -IP-pass—SUJB exams ’those who will not study will not pass the exams’ (70) Conditional a. ka -mu-laa-shom -a mu-laa-sing-a emitiani COND-Zp-IF -study-IND Zp-IF -pass-IND exams ’if you study you will pass the exams’ b. ka -mu-ta -a -shom -e ti -mu-sing-e mitiani COND-Zp-NEGZ-IP-study-SUBJ NEGl-Zp-pass-SUBJ exams ’if you don’t study you won’t pass the exams’ (71) Imperative: (i) indirect a. mugambile a -it -e embuzi elikwilagula tell him Is-slaughter-SUBJ goat black ’tell him to slaughter the black goat’ b. mugambile a -ta -it -a mbuzi elikwilagula tell him Is-NEGZ-slaughter-IND goat black ’tell him not to slaughter the black goat’ 106 (ii) direct a. kol-a omutiani kileki do -IND exam to-day '(you) take the exam to-day’ b. o -ta -kol-a mutiani kileki 25-NEG2-do -IND exam to-day ’you should not take the exam to-day.’ 'As before, in order to negate the remote future (RMF), the negative of the near future is used, with an adverbial phrase denoting the appropriate amount of time, as follows: (72) a. ba-lika-ib -a empia za ishebo 2 -RMF -steal-IND money of their father ’they will (in the far future) steal their fathers money.’ b. ba-ta -li-ib -a mpia za ba ishebo neila 2 -NEG2-NF-Steal-IND money of their father forever. ’they should never steal their fathers money ever.’ NEG2 is also used to negate a nominal infinitive: 107 (73) O -ku -ta -mal -a shule ku-a-ba-koz -a IV-INF-NEGZ-finish-IND school 15-2-do-CAUS-IND emilimo e -yo-mu-ta -li -kwend-a work IV-4 -2p-NEG2 -like -IND ’not finishing school has caused you to do the work you did not like’ An infinitive negated in this way may also be used as an imperative: (74) o -ta -ly -a! Zs-NEGZ-eat-IND ’(you) don’t eat’ 3.3.9.3 NEG3 NEG3 has the form Lag. It: appears in only one form, prefixed to «i, which in turn is attached to the stem, which is followed by the indicative morpheme. This verb form may be schematized as follows: NEG3 - kg - STEM - IND As illustrated in the following sets of examples, this form is used as a negative imperative: 108 (75) a. chumb-a amajuta galimu cook -IND oil be there cook! there is oil b. Ka -ku -0humb-a (amajuta ti-ga-limu) NEG3-UNR—cook -IND oil NEGl be in ’do not cook there is no oil’ (76) a. pim -a abana amaisho examine-IND children eyes examine the children’s eyes b. ka -ku -pima abana amaisho NEG3-UNR-examine children eyes ’do not examine the children’s eyes (e.g. when you know for sure you are not a doctor).’ It seems likely that the morpheme k_a__:_ is a shortened and cliticized form of the word leka, which may be translated as ’do not’ and is also used in negative imperatives. The following sets of examples contrast this negative imperative with the shortened form: (77) a. leka ku -lim -a alwazi do not INF-farm-IND rock ’do not farm on the rock.’ 109 b. ka ~ku ~lim -a alwazi NEG3—UNR-farm-IND rock ’do not farm on the rock’ (78) a. leka ~ku -tah -is -a omwana amaizi do not INF-draw water-CAUSE-IND child water 'do not make the child to draw some water’ b. ka -ku -tah -is -a omwana amaizi do not-UNR-draw-CAUS-IND child water ’do not cause the child to draw some water.’ 3.3.10. The Suffixal Morphemes In the preceding sections, we have seen that a verb stem in Haya may be inflected for mood, subject agreement, tense/aspect, finiteness, status as a relative, conditionality, focus and negation. The suffixal morphemes to be discussed in the next four sections constitutes a set of morphemes which are suffixed to inflected verb as a unit, without regard to its internal morphological content. 3.3.10.1. The Locative Morphemes There are three locative morphemes which are suffixed to the inflected verb. The first, :ha, indicates a location which is near, within the context of discourse: 110 (79) a. abashaija balaabileta-ha ebitebe men In Vb -LOC chairs ’the men will bring the chairs here (where I am standing)’ b. obagambile baije bashuntama-ha tell them to come In Vb -LOC ’tell them to come (and) sit here (e.g. where there are empty chairs).’ The second, -ku indicates, within the context of discourse, a location which is far. It is appropriate here too to mention that originally the locative kg is added to the verb from a separate word "oku" here, in which the initial vowel o- is deleted. (80) a. gambila abana bashuntama-oku (bashutamoku) tell children In Vb -LOC ’tell the children to sit there’ b. nababona mbagenda-oku (mbagendoku). I saw them In Vb‘ -LOC ’I saw them going there (lit: this side).’ In both examples there are phonological changes that take place as discussed in chapter 2 section 2.5.2. In order to clarify the qualifier "within the context of discourse," the 111 following examples may be cited in which, depending on the context, the same location may be referred to using either :11; or :1101: (81) a. AbaTanzania babili nibashomela-ha Marekani Tanzanians two In Vb -LOC America ’Two Tanzania are studying here in America.’ b. AbaTanzania babili nibashomela-oku (nibashomeloku) Tanzanians two In Vb -LOC Merekani America ’Two Tanzanians are studying ’here/there’ in America.’ The two examples above use two different locative morphemes within the same repeated sentence. The contextual meaning of the two differentiates the use of one form from the other.. In example (81.a), the speaker might be somebody who also lives in the United States, whereas example (81.b) may be uttered by a visitor from Tanzania who is presumably contextualizing the student residents in terms of distance of Tanzania to United States. However both the situations show some ambiguity in terms of distance depending on where the sentence was uttered and the location of these students in the U.S. The third locative morpheme, _:_m_u indicates location inside: 112 (82) abana bashomele-mu omukiyumba eki children In Vb-LOC in the room this ’the children should study in this room’ These three suffixal locative morphemes are cognate to the noun-class and agreement morphemes of classes 16, 17 and 18 in other Bantu languages.19 In Haya, only one of these classes, class 16, exists as a noun class (cf table 8). Some other Bantu languages such as Shona (Fortune 1955), Sotho (Doke and Mofokeng 1957) Tswana (Cole 1955) Zulu (Doke 1927) only to mention a few have all three classes. There is no trace of the other two locative noun-classes, apart from these three locative suffixal morphemes. This class is illustrated in the following examples, in which a Class 16 noun occurs 0 (83) a. Ha-elu ha-li-yo Omushana 16-out lG-be-REL sunshine ’there is sunshine outside’ b. ha-nu ha-in -a eibugumo 16-in-16—have-IND warmth ’it is warm in here’ 113 3.3.10.2. The Manner Morpheme The manner morpheme :ge is suffixed to the inflected verb immediately following the locative morpheme, if there is one. In a statement, this morpheme indicates that the action of the verb is performed well, or that the person or thing existing in the state denoted by the verb is also 2nd > lst Animacy hierarchy: Non-human > Human 119 Number hierarchy: Plural >.Singular Case hierarchy: Instrumental/Patient > Goal/Benefactive The relevance of each of these hierarchies to the order of Haya object markers will be discussed in turn. The operation of the person hierarchy in Haya may be seen in the following examples. In the (a) example of each pair, the object marker referring to the person hierarchy appears closer to the stem; the (b) example shows that the reverse order is ungrammatical: (93) a. Ba-ku-m -p -el -a ebitooke. 2 ~23—Is-give-APL-IND bananas ’They give you bananas for me.’ ’They give me bananas for you.’ b. *Ba-m-ku-p-el-a ebitooke. (94) a. Ba-mu-ku-m -p —el -a ebitooke. 2 —3s-Zs-Is-give-APL-IND bananas ’They give him bananas for you.’ *’They give you bananas for him.’ b. *Ba-ku-mum-p-el-a ebitooke. In these examples, the object. markers contrast only in person. but are constant in terms of animacy, case and number. In this way, only the effects of the person 120 hierarchy have been tested. However, it can also be shown that when the requirements of the person hierarchy conflict with those of the other hierarchies, the person hierarchy takes precedence. The following two sets of examples indicate that the person hierarchy takes precedence over the number hierarchy, as in (95), and also over the case hierarchy, as in (96) (note some phonological changes): (95) a. Ba-mu-tu-h -el -a ebitooke. 2 -3s-Ip-giveeAPL-IND bananas ’They give him bananas for us.’ ’They give us bananas for him.’ b. *Ba—tu-mu-h -el -a ebitooke. 2 -Ip-3s-give-APL-IND bananas (96) a. A-ka -mu-ku-leet -el -a. I-RMP-3s-23-bring-APL-IND ’He brought him to you.’ ’He brought you to him.’ b. *A-ka -ku-mu-leet -el -a I-RMP—Zs-3s-bring-APL-IND The conclusion that the person hierarchy takes precedence over the case hierarchy may be drawn from the observation that the order of the object markers in examples (95) and 121 (96), (the set contrasting person. and. case) may' not be varied to reflect case contrasts. Instead, the single possible order is ambiguous between both case interpretations, so that either object marker may refer to either case. It would also be desirable to contrast the person and animacy hierarchies in this way. However, this contrast is not testable, because the relevant example would require an object marker referring to an inanimate object which is ranked higher on the person hierarchy than an object marker referring to an animate object: i.e., the inanimate object would also have to be first or second person, and inanimates cannot be referred to in either the first or second person in Haya. Nevertheless, with some complications, it can also be shown that the animacy hierarchy takes precedence over the number hierarchy in the same way as the person hierarchy does. The following set of examples contrasts the animacy hierarchy with the number hierarchy: (97) a. A -ki-ba-gul-il -a Is-7 -2 -buy-APL-IND ’He buys it for them.’ #’He buys them for it.’ Note that in this example, the second interpretation is ruled out for pragmatic reasons (an inanimate object is 122 unlikely to have anything bought for it), contrary to in example (96) above. However, reversing the order of the object markers, in violation of the animacy hierarchy, produces an ungrammatical sentence: b. * A -ba-ki-gul-il -a. Is-Z -7 -buy-APL-IND We will say for this example that it is ungrammatical because it violates the animacy hierarchy. However, it could also be ungrammatical because of the case hierarchy, since the object marker referring to the patient is closer to the verb stem than the object marker referring to the applicative (benefactive). The opposite interpretation, which does not violate the case hierarchy is, as we have seen, ruled out on pragmatic grounds. The evidence is indeterminate in this case, and such indeterminacy cannot be eliminated from this discussion. In any case, the example indicates that the number hierarchy may be overridden. Total indeterminacy is found when the animacy hierarchy is contrasted with the case hierarchy. (98) a. ba-ka -gi-mu-twal-a -mu 2 -RMP-9 -3s-take-IND-LOC ’They sent him to it (9; i.e., the cave)’ #’They sent it to him.’ 123 As in the above examples, the second interpretation is ruled out for pragmatic reasons (a cave cannot be sent somewhere). However, as before, reversing the order of the object markers, in violation of the animacy hierarchy, produces an ungrammatical sentence: b. * ba-ka -mu-gi-twal-a -mu 2 -RMP-3s-9 -take-IND-LOC This example could be ruled. out by either the animacy hierarchy or the case hierarchy, since both are violated and this example does not distinguish the two. The examples we have seen so far indicate one hierarchy taking absolute precedence over another: an. ordering of object markers which contradicts the hierarchy is ungrammatical. However, when the number of case hierarchies are compared, the effects of each are relative rather than absolute. The number hierarchy has the effect of placing an object. marker (which refers to) a singular* noun phrase) closer to the verb stem than an object marker referring to a plural noun phrase, as seen in the following example: (99) a. A -ka -ba-mu-leet -el -a 3s-RMP-2p-33-bring-APL-IND ’He brought them to him’ ’He brought him to them’ 124 In this example, the singular object marker occurs closer to the verb stem than the plural object marker, in accordance with the number hierarchy. Note that these object markers refer to two thematic arguments of the verb: the patient (what is being brought) and the goal (who the patient is being brought to). As Shown ill the translations, either object marker may refer to either thematic argument. Now consider the following example, in which the object markers of the previous example appear in the opposite order, in violation of the number hierarchy. b. A -ka-mu-ba-leet -el -a he-P3-3s—Zp-bring-APL-IND ’He brought him to them’ *’He brought them to him’ Here only one interpretation is possible, in. which the plural object marker closer to the stem is the goal. This example indicates that the number hierarchy may be violated when the results conform to the requirements of the case hierarchy. The interpretation of ’example (99.b) in which both the number and the case hierarchies are violated is not possible. Note as well that the case hierarchy is violated in (99.a) but this violation is possible only when the number hierarchy has been obeyed. 125 These examples indicate that both the number and the case hierarchies are relevant to the order of object markers in Haya, but only in a relative sense. If number were completely irrelevant, then (99.a), in which number is obeyed but case violated, should be ungrammatical, but it is not. If case were completely irrelevant then the second interpretation of (99.b) should be possible, but it is not, it is precisely when both number and case are violated that complete unacceptability results. The data we have presented in this section suggest that, of the four hierarchies, the effects of person and animacy are absolute, whereas the effects of number and case are cumulative. This observation in turn suggests that when the animacy and case hierarchies are in conflict it is in fact the animacy hierarchy that takes precedence, even though this effect is not directly observable. 3.4.2. The Reflexive Marker The reflexive marker in Haya occupies the same position immediately preceding the verb stem as the object markers. It signifies coreferentiality between the subject and the object of a sentence. Unlike ‘the object. markers,. the reflexive marker takes the invariant form :e;, subject to phonological changes described in chapter 2 (section 2.5.2). Also, a verb stem may contain only one reflexive marker, and no object markers when the reflexive marker is present. The reflexive marker may be exemplified as follows: 126 (100) a. abana ba-e -shal-a children 2 -RFM-cut -IND ’the children have cut themselves’ b. abojo ba-li -e -mois —a boys 2 -RFM-shave-IND ’the boys will shave themselves.’ 3.5. The Verbal Extensions As mentioned above in section 3.2, a verb stem may consist of a verb root plus one or more morphemes which modify its lexical meaning and/or alter its argument structure. These morphemes are usually referred to as the verbal extensions, and this section will briefly describe and exemplify their use.23 (See Byarushengo et al. 1977, Scotton 1967, Khamisi 1983, Maw 1984). .A single root may contain more than one extension, each of which typically contributes to the total meaning of the stem, with certain cooccurrence restrictions which will be discussed in the next chapter. In this chapter, the meaning of each extension will be illustrated in isolation. In the absence of other extensions, each extension is suffixed to the root, directly preceding the final vowel, schematized as follows: (101) ROOT - EXT - IND SUBJ 127 When more than one extension is present, all are suffixal, but they are ordered with respect to each other in ways that will be described in the next chapter. 3.5.1. The Causative Extension The causative extension in Haya takes one of the following forms: gig-z—es- or -iz-/-ez- (subject to vowel harmony, described in chapter 2, section 2.5.2.), 3;; or :y;.3‘ In order to describe the meaning which the causative extension contributes to the verb stem, we adopt Ashton’s (1944: 232) description for Swahili, which is "to" express causation." Nevertheless, as. will be seen below, while a definition such as this one may cover most of the uses of the causative, it does not cover all of them. The causative extension. may .be exemplified in the following pairs of examples, in which the first example contains an unextended verb and the second contains the same verb plus the causative extension:23 (102) a. Abana ba-a -bon-a ente children 2 -IP-see-IND cow ’the children have seen the cow’ b. Abana ba-a -bon-es -a nyina-bo ente children 2 -IP-see-CAUS-IND mother their cow ’the children have shown their mother a cow’ (lit. caused their mother to see a cow) 128 (103) a. Omulilo gu-a -yak -a fire 3 -IP-blaze-IND ’the fire has blazed’ b. abana ba-a -yak —y -a omulilo children 2 -IP-blaze-CAUS-IND fire ’the children have set a fire’ (104) a. Abakozi ba-laa-kol-a emilimo mingi workers 2 -IF -do -IND work many ’the workers will do a lot of work’ b. Abebembezi ba-laa-kol -z +a abakozi emilino leaders 2 —IF -work-CAUS-IND workers work mingi many ’the leaders will make the workers do a lot of work’ The following use of the causative has the effect of adding an object referring to the instrument of the action: (105) a. Abafungwa ba-li-lim-a eshamba Prisoners 2-NF-cultivate-IND farm ’the prisoners will cultivate the farm (e.g. to- morrow)’ 129 b. Abafungwa ba-li-lim -is -a eshamba Prisoners 2 -NF-cultivate-CAUS-IND farm etarakita tractor ’the prisoners will cultivate the farm with a tractor’ Note that if the causative extension were not present, the instrument could only appear in the sentence as a prepositional phrase: (106) Abafungwa balilima eshamba nametargkiti ’prisoners will cultivate the farm withma tragtgr’ muc- 3.5.2. The Applied Extension The applied extension has also been referred to as benefactive extension (Byarushengo et al. 1977 for Haya) and as prepositional extension (Ashton 1944 for Swahili). In Haya, it takes the form -' - - -, depending on vowel harmony. It adds an object to the argument structure of the verb; without the extension, this object would have to be expressed in a prepositional phrase, or else be unexpressable. This object may represent a number of thematic roles, three of which are mentioned here: 130 (1) An applied verb may denote an action in relation to a person, normally for his/her advantage: (107) a. abaalimu ba-ka -leet -a amaua teachers 2 -RMP-bring-IND flowers ’the teacher brought flowers’ b. abaalimu ba-ka -leet —el -a abanafunzi babo teachers 2 -RMP-bring—APL-IND students their amaua flowers ’the teachers brought some flowers for/to their students’ Note that there is no example possible in which the verb is unextended and the beneficiary expressed in a propositional 'phrase. The applied object may also represent an "anti- beneficiary." This can be exemplified by a sentence like. (108) a. Omushuma owo bakwete a -ka -ba-f -el -a the thief whom they caught Is-RMP-Z -die-APL-IND omumikono yabo hands theirs ’the thief whom they caught died in their hands’ 131 This sentence may be interpreted as "anti-beneficiary" unless the catchers wanted their thief to die before taking him to court, a propasal which is highly unlikely. (ii) An applied object may denote a place. (109) a. ente e-ka -f -a cow 9-RMP-die-IND ’the cow died’ b. ente e -ka -f -el -a omulweya cow IV-RMP-die-APL-IND pasture ’the cow died in the pasture’ (iii) An applied object may' denote a cause, reason or motive: (110) a. Richard, ba-lika-mu-fung-a Richard 2 -RMF -2p-jail-IND ’Richard, they will jail him’ b. Richard ba-lika-mu-fung-il -a obushuma Richard 2 -RM -23-jail-APL-IND robbery ’they will jail Richard for robbery’ 132 3.5.3. The Passive Extension .The passive extension in Haya takes the form ;E; (with one irregular exception, to be noted below). In a sentence in which the verb contains the passive extension, the object of the unextended verb is the subject, and the subject of the unextended verb appears optionally in a prepositional phrase, exemplified as follows: (111) a. embwa y-a -lum -a .abashuma dog 9-Ip-bite-IND robbers ’the dog bite the robbers’ b. abashuma ba-a -lum -w -a embwa thieves 2 -IP-bite-PASS-IND dog ’the robbers were bitten by the dog’ (112) a. Omushaija y -a -mu-tel-a man Is-Ip-3s-hit-IND ’the man hit him’ b. n -a -tel-w -a omushaija Is-IP-hit-PASS-IND man ’I was hit (by) the man’ Note that when an agent is human there is a preference not to have a propositional phrase even if it is possible as exemplified in 113. 133 (113) a. abashuma baalumwa na embwa ’the robbers were bitten by the dog’ b. natelwa na omushaija ’I was hit by the man’ The following exemplifies an irregular form of the passive extension: (114) abana bato ba-h -abw -a ebitoke na children young 2 —give-PASS-IND bananas by nyinenkuru bo grandmothers their ’the children are given bananas by their grandmother’ In addition, there are verbs which are used exclusively in the passive voice with active meanings, as observed by Rascher (1958:39): (115) a. abalimu ba-yeb -w -a omukutano teachers 2 -forget-PASS-IND meeting ’the teachers forgot the meeting’ b. omukazi ogu a-tamw-a abagenyi woman this hate visitors ’this woman hates visitors’ 134 3.5.4. The Reciprocal Extension The reciprocal extension in Haya takes the form -a -—. This extension changes the meaning of'ti verb to indicate that the action is reciprocal or mutual; i.e., done to "one 'another." The reciprocal extension may be exemplified as follows: (116) a. Halima n -a -chuchul-a Farida eishoke Halima Is-IP-comb -IND Farida hair ’Halima is combing Farida’s hair’ b. Halima na Farida ni -ba—chuchul-angan-a eishoke FOO-2 -comb -RCP -IND hair ’Halima and Farida are combing one another’s hair’ Because of its meaning, a sentence containing a verb with the reciprocal extension is well-formed only if the verb‘ denotes an action which may be performed mutually and has a plural subject. The following pair of examples are ungrammatical because, in (a), the intransitive verb does not denote an action which may be performed mutually and, in (b) the verb has a singular subject: (117) a. *Abaisiki ba-laa -shabuk-angan-a. omwiga girls 2 -IF -cross -RCP -IND river ’the girls will cross one another the river.’ 135 b. *Omwisiki n -a -chuchul-angan-a eishoke girl Is-IP-comb -RCP -IND hair t’The girl is combing one another the hair.’ Even if a verb is transitive, and has a plural subject, its use as a reciprocal may be barred pragmatically.25 (118) abashaija aba ba-a -chumb-angan-a men these 2 -Ip-cook -RCP -IND ’these men are cooking ’one another” Example (118) is acceptable only when it is enterpreted idiomatically as exemplified in (117). (119) Abashaija aba ba-a-chumb-angan-a kukola ebyashala men these 2-IP MOTIVATE-RCP-IND to-do business ’these men have motivated one another to do business.’ 3.5.5. The Repetitive Extension The term "repetitive extension" is used in this work to refer to three extensions which express various forms of intensity, including repeated, frequent, continuous or forceful action. These extensions are: 1) -' - - 2- (subject to vowel harmony), 2) :aij; and 3) :ul;, and may be exemplified as follows: (120) (121) (122) 136 Koku a -shon-a emyenda Koku Is-sew -IND clothes ’Koku sews clothes’ Koku a ~shon-elez-a emyenda Koku Is-sew -RPT -IND clothes ’Koku sews clothes repeatedly’ Bojo ba-gamb-ile ba-angu —e tugende please 2 -tell-RP 2 -quick-SUBJ (so that) we go ’please tell them to be quick so that we go’ Bojo bagambile ba-angu -iliz-e tugende please tell them 2 -quick-RPT -SUBJ (so that) we go ’please tell them to travel fast so that we go’ Omwana ogu a—li-nag -a ebyakula omunju child this I-NF-throw-IND food in the house ’this child will throw the food in the house’ Omwana ogu a-li-nag -aij-a ebyakulya omunju child this I-NF-throw-RPT-IND food in the house ’this child will throw the food continuously/ carelessly in the house’ 137 (123) a. abashuma ba-ka -sik -a ebiliibwa thieves 2 -RMP-pull-IND cassava ’the thieves pulled the cassava’ b. abashuma ba-ka -sik -ul -a ebiliibwa thieves 2 -RMP-pull-RPT-IND cassava ’the thieves pulled forcefully/hurriedly the cassava’ Since these three morphemes have a common strand of meaning, we have decided to treat them as a single extension in this work. In this respect, we follow Rascher (1958:84), who uses the word "intensive" to refer to this set of morphemes for Haya, and Doke (1943:22) who uses the term "extensive" to refer to different forms appearing in ‘various Bantu languages. 131 Zulu (Doke 1927:148) the form is -isis- suffixed to the verb-stem. However, it is possible that future research will suggest that each of these morphemes is associated most often with a particular aspect of intensity, and. such. an .association would. provide justification for assuming that they constitute separate verbal extensions. For example, -' ' - - - may turn out to be the only one of the three which consistently denotes repeated action (as opposed to forceful action). For now, however, we will assume the present analysis. 138 3.5.6. The Stative Extension The stative extension in Haya takes the form -ik-/ek-, subject to vowel harmony. Its usage is similar to that of the passive: in a sentence in which the verb contains'the stative extension, the object of the unextended verb is the subject of the stative verb. However, there are two significant differences. First, in a sentence containing a stative verb, the subject of the unextended verb may not appear, in a prepositional phrase or in any other form. Second, another nuance of meaning which distinguishes the stative from the passive is that of potentiality: the subject of the stative verb is implied to be capable of undergoing the action denoted by the unextended verb. For this reason, the stative extension is readily translatable using the English adjectival suffix .1832.qu The stative extension may be exemplified as follows: (124) a. e -nsi e-gi ° t -a -11m -ik-a (telimika) IV-land IV-this NEG-IP-cultivate-ST-IND ’this land is uncultivatible’ b. e -bi-kombe bi-ona bi-a -yat -ik-a (byatika) IV-8 cup 8 -all 8 -IP-break-ST-IND ’all cups are breakable’ 139 c. a -ma-banga ti -ga-temb —ek-a bwangu IV-6 -mountain NEG-6 -climb-ST-IND quick ’the mountains are not quickly climbable’ Other scholars have used the term Neuter (e.g. Doke 1927, 1943, Doke and Mofokeng 1957, Cole 1955 to mention a few) to refer to the stative extension (a term used by both Rascher (1958) and Byarushengo (1977)). 3.6. Conclusion This chapter’ has described the .meanings and linear order of the 23 morphemes which occur in the various forms of the Haya ‘verb. The next. chapter will describe the cooccurrence restrictions holding between these morphemes. 140 CHAPTER 3 ENDNOTES 1 The reversive morpheme will not be discussed further. It is in this work treated as unproductive in Haya (see appendix B) and thus regarded as lexical. See also Khamisi (1983:94) on the treatment of reversive (called conversive) as a lexical group in Swahili also a Bantu language. Also in order to help readers’ confusion in the use of terms, compared to other writers, we list the terms we used along with other different terms in different studies on the grammar of African languages. This list contains only the frequently used terms in this work (individual authors will be mentioned as we discuss the concepts). Reversive: Conversive (Ashton 1954, Khamisi 1983) Benefective: Applied, Prepositional Repetitive: Augmentative Reciprocal: Associative Stative: Neuter (Rascher 1958) 4 Several writers including Inn; not limited to Byarushengo 1977, Maw 1985, Perez (Harford) 1986, have used the term verbal extensions. 4 In Haya society a woman gives birth for the man; that is the society practises the patriarch system. 4 This term is not used for these morphemes in Haya or in any other Bantu language in any source that we know of. We have taken the term from Bybee; of chapter 5 for justification. 141 5 See Ashton et al. (1954:18) quoting the definition from Doke’s Bantu Linguistic Terminology in footnote 1. 4 From now on, subject morphemes will be indicated by numbers corresponding to their noun classes. As regards noun class 1 and 2, first and second person singular will be indicated by a number e.g. 23 where 2 is for the second person and s for singular while p will be for plural. 7 Sabimana 1986 argues convincingly that noun classes 15, 16 and 17 denoting place in Kirundi (locatives), a language closely related to Haya language are not noun classes (class markers) for the following reasons: i noun. class markers as Sabimana. calls them are selected by at least one stem, but not locatives ii locatives never trigger agreement as do other markers iii Normal class markers attach to stems but locatives can attach to proper names and personal pronouns iv They do not show the same alternations in the same environments as other class markers do. Thus Sabimana suggests they be treated as prepositions, a suggestion which we did not incorporate in this study. 4 The indicative and subjunctive morphemes have also been referred to as mood suffixes with reference to Swahili (Vitale 1981:17), and also as final vowel (Perez (Harford) 1985:10)) for Kikuyu and Shond. 4 The number of tenses will be modified after discussing the focus morpheme in section (3.3.9). 142 1° We were unable to obtain Moshi’s full version of her paper presented at the 19th African Linguistics at Boston University April 14-17, 1988. We only had access to her outline distributed at the conference and a personal conversation with the author. 11 In his treatment of Kikuyu, Clements (1984:293) treats -ire as having a final vowel 1;, so that the morpheme itself is -dJ~u Such an analysis has not been adopted in this work. Note also that [l] and [r] are allophones of the same phoneme /l/ in most Bantu languages. See chapter 2 (14). Kikuyu :13; is cognate to Haya :ilg. 13 The habitual morpheme -ag- in Haya is cognate to ~ag- in Kikuyu (Barlow 195) where it is used to form three past imperfect tenses: "immediate past imperfect," "near past imperfect” and "remote past imperfect." Barlow notes (ibid:142) that when used in. past time in. general, it expresses (like in Haya) an action done habitually, or recurrently . . . .Other Bantu languages which use this form as a habitual morpheme are among others Sukuma, Nyamwezi (personal conversation with Kahigi). Furthermore, recently Wilt (1988:58 first draft) notes a very suggestive-looking verbal morpheme -ak- in Bukavu Swahili, which may be a loan from ethnic Bantu languages. 13 We also note Polome’s 1976:77 suggestion that the :31; nominal agentive suffix in standard Swahili is cognate to ‘;ag; expressing a constantly repeated or a habitual state. 143 14 In Haya society a man marries and a woman is married, therefore if example 46.b were to be interpreted in the Haya context, it would have read "leave him alone, he will marry when he will like." 15 Compound tenses are not discussed in ‘this work. However, they have mostly been covered in the discussion of the simple tenses. For details of these tenses refer to Nurse 1979, Byarushengo 1976, Hyman and Byarushengo 1984 in Clements and Goldsmith (eds) p. 53-104. 14 The IV has also been called variously a "double prefix" (Guthrie 1974), a "noun augment" (de Blois 1970), and pre-prefix (Masamba 1976). 17 When an object is relativized, the relative clause is marked with a separate relative marker, and the form of the verb is not changed from its non-relative form except for tonality which is realized as log (L) on the object (See Hyman and. Byarushengo 1984:79 in. Clements and Goldsmith (eds) 1984). 14 Barlow 1951:117 calls the focus morpheme the initial marker n;;. 1’ Classes 16, 17 and 18 are numbers assigned to the three locative noun classes in the Welmers (1973) numbering system. See also section (3.3.10.1). 4° The question morpheme :ki constitutes an exception to the generalization mentioned in the introduction to section 3.3.10, that suffixal morphemes are attached to the inflected verb as a unit, without regard to its internal 144 morphological content. This morpheme can, in fact, only be suffixed to transitive verbs, an observation that will be taken up in the next chapter (section 4.6). Therefore, in order to attach this morpheme correctly, it is necessary to know the argument structure of the root and how this argument structure has been affected by verbal extensions.) 31 Note that object. markers may’ co-occur ‘with post verbal object NPS in Haya: a. a -laa-gi-mu-gul-il -a ishe emotoka Is-IF -4 -3s-hit-APL-IND father car ’he will buy it for him the father a car’ b. embwa zi-lika-ba-lum -a Amina na Fatuma dogs 10-RMF -2 -bite-IND Amina and Fatuma ’the dogs will (in the far future) bite them Amina and Fatuma.’ The co-occurrence of the OM and object NPs is unusual but when it happens, the object NP(s) function as a topic, a point further discussed in chapter 5. 3* Bantu languages vary as to how many object markers are permitted within a single form. Swahili for instance permits only one OM as is the case of Kikuyu (Barlow 1951). Other Bantu languages such as Nkore and Kiga (Taylor 1985) languages of Uganda permit more than one OMs. ' 33 Only productive extensions are considered: writers on Bantu languages tend to take anything that appears on a couple of verbs with roughly the same meaning and elevate it to the rank of extensions, but we will not do this here. 145 34 This is not meant to be the maximal forms of causatives in Haya, nor are all uses discussed. *5 Literal translations may be acceptable in folktales, like the Haya folktale where the Hare and the Tortoise are supposed to cook each other. This suggests that verbs with ridiculous meanings may nonetheless exist under exceptional contexts of usage. 146 Table 6. The Internal Structure of Conjugated Verb in Haya 1 Initial Vowel IV 2 Conditional COND 3 Negative NEG 4 Focus FOC 5 Subject Marker SM 6 Tense TNS 7 Infinitive INF 8 Reflexive RFM 9a Direct Object (Accusative) DO 9b Indirect Object (Dative) IO 10 VERB ROOT ROOT 11 Reversive RV 12 Causative CAUS 13 Applied APL 14 Stative ST 15 Passive PASS 16 Reciprocal RCP 17 Repetitive RPT 18 Indicative IND 19 Subjunctive SUBJ 20 Locative 9 LOC 21 Manner particle MNR 22 Interrogative (Question) Q 23 Intensifier INT 147 Table 7: Inventory of Morphemes and Allomorphs. Morpheme Allomorphs 1. IV a/-012, 016, 0112, 0116 e/-cl4, 015, 017, 018, 019, 0110 o/-cll, 013, 0111, 0113, 0114, 0115, 0118 2. COND ka- 3. NEG ti/-c{main‘clause} ta/-{other} ka/-INF 4. FOC ni- 5. SM a/-cll, ba/-012, gu/-cl3, e/-cl4, 019, li/-015, gal-0116, ki/-cl7, bi/-018, zi/-cllO, lu/-clll, ka/-0112, tu/-0113, bu/-cll4, ku/-cll5, 17, ha/-0116, mu/-cll8. Numbers correspond to noun-class numbering system. 6. TNS puH/-, STL/-kya-, IPl-a, NPS/—a--ile, RP/-ile, RMP/-ka-, FRP/-la--ile, IPH/-a--ag-, IF/-laa-, NF/-li-, RMF/-lika-. 7. INF ku- 8. RFM —e- 9. DO/IO (OM): m/-cll:1, ku/-cll:2,15,17, mu/-cll:3, 2:2, 18, tu/-2:1,13, ba/-012:3, gu/-013, gi/-cl4,9, li/-cl5, gal-016, ki/-cl7, bi/-cl8, zi/-cllO, lu/-0111, ka/-0112, bul-cll4, ha/-0116. 10. ROOT (STEM) varies 11. RV -ulul-/-olol- 12. CAUS -is-/-es, -y-, -z-, -s-. 13. APL -il-/-el 14. ST -ik-/—ek 15. PASS -w— 16. RCP -angan- 17. RPT -iliz/-elez, -aij-, -ul— 18. IND -a- 19. SUBJ - -e 20. LOC -ha-/-ku—/-mu- 21. MNR -ge ' 22. Q -ki-/-li- 23. INT -kwo This list includes vowel harmony as discussed in chapter 2. 148 Table 8: List of Noun Classes and Subject Morpheme Markers. Class Noun prefix Person Number Subject Marker 1 Mu- 1 SG n- 2 SG 0- 3 SG a- 2 BA- 1 PL tu- 2 PL mu- 3 PL ba- 3 Mu- SG gu- 4 M1- PL e- 5 i-/Li SG li- 6 MA- PL 8a- 7 KI- SG ki-I 8 BI- PL b1- 9 N- SG e- 10 Lu- SG lu- 11 N- PL zi 12 KA- SG ka- 13 Tu- SG/PL tu- 14 Bu- 7 PL bu- 15 Ku- SG ku- 16 HA- - - ha- 17 - - - 18 - - - Table 9: Haya Affiraative and Negative Tenses 1.4E3 Affiraative Tense Harker Exanple Gloss Present Universal Habitual (PUH) - ba-lua-a ’they usually bite’ Still Tense (STL) -kya- zi-kya-nyw-a ’they (cows) are still drinking’ Iaaadiate Past (IP) -a- ba-a-ching-a ’they have closed’ Near Past (NPS) -a- -ile tu-a-zin-ila ’we have sung’ Recent Past (RP) - -ila gu-gw-ile ’it (tree) fell’ Reaota Past (RHP) -ka- ba-ka-ib-a ’they stole’ Far Renata Past (FRP) -la- -ila e-la-gw-ile ’they (trees) have already fell (long ago)’ Iaaadiata Past Habitual (IPH) -a- -a9- ba-a-tea-ag-a ’they were habitually cutting’ Twaadiata Future (IF) -laa- ga-laa-yatik-a ’thay (eggs) will break’ Near Future (NF) -li- a-li-lua-a ’ha will bita’ Relate Future (RHF) -lika- o-lika-jua-a ’You will in the resale future abusa.’ Negative Tense Barker Exaapla Gloss Present Universal Habitual (PUH) - ti-ba-lua-a ’they do not usually bita.’ Still Tense (STL) -kya- ti-zi-ki-nyw-a ’they (cows) are no longer drinking’ laaadiata Past (IP) -a- ti-ba-a-ching-a ’they have not closed’ Near Past (NPS) -a- -ila ti-tu-ka-zin-ila ’wa have not sung’ Recent Past (RP) - -ila ti-gu-gw-ila ’it (tree) did not fall’ Reacts Past (RHP) -ka- ti-ba-ka-ib-ag-a ’they have never stola’ Far Raaota Past (FRP) -la- -ile ta-Ra-gw-ag-a ’they trees have never fall’ Iaaadiata Past Habitual (IPH) -a- -ag- ti-ba-a-teI-ag-a ’they were habitually not cutting’ laaediata Future (IF) -laa- ti-ga-yatik-a ’thay (eggs) will not break’ Near Future (NF) -11- t-a-li-luI-a ’ha will never bite’ Raaota Future ' (RHF) -lika- t-o-li-qu-a neila ’you will never ever abusa’ Table 10: Subject Relative Horpheees 1.5() Class IV SH Exaeple Gloss 1 a- a a-a-gend-a (he) who does e.g. ean 2 a- -ba aba-gend-a (they) who go e.g. een 3 o- -gu o-gu-laa-gw-a (it) which will fall e.g. tree 4 e- -e e-e-laa-gw-a (they) which will fall e.g. trees 5 e- -li e-li-kya-lil-a (it) which is still crying e.g. eye 6 a- -ga a-ga-kya-lil-a (they) which are still crying e.g. eyes 7 e- -ki e-ki-a-bul-a (it) which got lost e.g. book 8 e- -bi e-bi-a-bul-a (they) which got lost e.g. books 9 e- -e e-e-li-bul-a (it) which will grow e.g. cow 10 e- -zi ezi-li-kul-a (they) which will grow e.g. cows 11 o- -lu o-lu-laa-ij-a (it) which will coee e.g. death 12 a- -ka a-ka-li-chueb-w-a (it) which will be cooked e.g. little banana 13 o- -tu o-tu-laa-gul-w-a (they) which will be bought e.g. little things 14 o- -bu o-bu-a-sheshek-a which has poured e.g. flour 15 o- -ku o-ku-laa-gue-a (it) which will becoee hard e.g. cooking 16 a- -ha a-ha-laa-sheeel-a (it) which will be good e.g. place 17 - ku - 18 - eu - 151 Table 11: Direct/Indirect Objects. Noun Class Person Number Objects: (DO/IO) 1 1 86 -m- 2 SG -ku- 3 SG -mu- 2 1 PL -tu- 2 PL -mu- 3 PL -ba- 3 SG -gu— 4 PL -81- 5 ' SG -li- 6 ~ PL -ga- 7 SG A -ki- 8 PL -bi- 9 SG -gi- 10 SG -lu- 11 PL -zi- 12 SG -ka- 13 . SG -tu- 14 ’ PL -bu- 15 ' SG -ku- 16 - _-ha- 17 - ' -ku- 18 - -mu- CHAPTER 4 THE MORPHEMES OF THE VERB: COOCCURRENCE RESTRICTIONS 4.1. Introduction Chapter 3 described the linear order of the verbal morphemes with little comment on cooccurrence restrictions which hold between these morphemes. This chapter will focus on two topics: 1) what cooccurrence restrictions there are, and 2) what, if any, reasons (syntactic, semantic, pragmatic, phonological or arbitrary) can be given for these restrictions. As observed at the end of chapter 3, the total number of morphemes which constitute the various forms of the Haya verb come to 23. If all of these morphemes were freely combinable with each other, the total number of forms in the Haya verbal paradigm would come to (1,334,832). However, as was apparent in the previous.chapter, the number of actually occurring forms is a small fraction of the possible forms. (This ratio of possible to actual forms will be discussed in the next chapter, section 5.4.) This observation raises the problem of determining which of the possible forms are actual, and thereby determining the cooccurrence restrictions which hold among the verbal morphemes. Two methods were used to derive the 152 153 generalizations cited in this chapter. First, some of these generalizations were discovered by introspection by the author, a first-language speaker of Haya” Others were discovered by application of the following method: the morphemes of the Haya verb were numbered sequentially, from left to right. Then, random sets of these numbers were selected and verb forms composed using just the morphemes corresponding to these numbers in the appropriate order. The resulting forms were then evaluated to uncover generalizations about cooccurrence restrictions that may have escaped introspection. The presentation in the remainder of the chapter is organized. as follows. First, the verbal morphemes are divided into three groups, based on their position wdthin the verb: 1) prestem: preceding the stem; 2) poststem: following the stem; and 3) stem: stem-internal. Second, the cooccurrence restrictions for the prestem morphemes are discussed, then, the cooccurrence restrictions for the poststem morphemes, then, the cooccurrence restrictions for the stem morphemes. The discussion of the non-stem morphemes presupposes that they may cooccur with an unanalyzed stem, and the discussion of the stem morphemes presupposes that the occurrence of non-stem morphemes is irrelevant to the internal structure of the stem. The few exceptions to these generalizations will be discussed last, and commented on further in chapter 5. 154 4.2. Cooccurrence Restrictions of Prestem Morphemes 4.2.1. The Subject Marker The subject marker is required in every verb form of the Haya verbal paradigm except the indicative imperative (cf. section 3.3.1.) and the infinitive. The following examples indicate that it may not appear in the two latter forms: (1) * tu-ku -shun -a Ip-INF-pinch-IND x ’We to pinch you’ The infinitive morpheme kg in example (1) has the same shape and occupies the same slot as the object morpheme 3,5,3: second person singular as discussed earlier. When $2 in (1) is regarded as the OM second person singular then the form in (1) repeated here as (2) is well formed. (2) tu-ku-shun -a Ip-Zs-pinch-IND ’We pinch you’ Similarly example (3) is ungrammatical as an imperative; otherwise possible as a present universal habitual (24). 155 (3) * ba-shom-a 2 -read-IND (imperative) (4) ba-shom-a 2 -read-IND ’they (habitually) study’ Example (3) is well formed only as an imperative without the subject morpheme (2) as shown in (5) (5) shom-a read-IND ’study’ Furthermore example (3) is also well-formed only when used with the subjunctive morpheme to be discussed in detail in section (4.3.2.). We however give an example here: (6) ba-shom-e 2 -read-SUBJ ’let them study’ We now, discuss the cooccurrence restrictions of the infinitive (INF) with other prestem morphemes. 156 4.2.2. The Infinitive Morpheme The infinitive morpheme may cooccur only with the poststem indicative marker and NEG2. It may not cooccur with the subject marker: (7) a. ku -zin -a INF-sing-IND ’to sing’ b. * a -ku -zin -a Is-INF-sing-IND ’he to sing.’ Example (7.a) indicates the. use' of the INF ‘without the subject marker and (7.b) shows that the two may not cooccur. As was the case with the subject marker above in section 4.2.1, the same reason applies here. That is, the marker - ku- occupies the same slot and has the same shape as the object marker (OM) second person singular. This might be regarded as a phonological identity restriction, a term we are using here to mean the cooccurrence restrictions of two or more morphemes which have similar phonological values. The infinitive morpheme may not occur with any tense marker: (8) a. t e -li—ka -ku -gw -a IV-9 -RMP-INF-fall-IND 157 ...... 2p- -INF pierce- -IND c. * zi-ku -li-gomok -a IO-INF-NF-become fat-IND d. * ba-ka -ku -ib -a 2 -RMP-INF-steal-IND Examples (8.a) and (8.b) indicate that no tense marker precede the INF morpheme and examples (8.0) and (8.d) indicate that no tense marker may follow the INF morpheme. However, in other Bantu languages such as Shona (9.a) and Kikuyu (9.b) (Harford (Perez) 1986: 68) infinitives have "overt future markers as illustrated in the following examples from Shona and Kikuyu: (9) a. Ku -cha-ziva ' INF-fut-know ’to know still’ (Fortune 1980: 3.60) (Shona) b. Menya u -gok -a gu -ka negenia. know 23 fut. come ind. inf. fut. Is obj unbraid (kikuyu) (Barlow 1951: 127). ’Mind you do not come to upbraid me (i.e., at some future time).’ 158 The infinitive morpheme cannot co-occur with the subjunctive morpheme as shown below: (10) a. * ku -gend-e INF-go -SUBJ b. * ku -lis -e ente INF-graze-IND cattle c. * ku -ly -e omuchele INF-eat-SUBJ rice The cooccurrence restriction holding between the INF, and SUBJ is because of the cooccurrence requirement of the SUBJ morpheme with the SM (see 4.3.2 for details). We have also seen that SM and INF do not co-occur. Thus there is a clash of the occurrence requirements of SUBJ with SM and cooccurrence restrictions of INF with SM. The infinitive morpheme k1; cannot co-occur with NEGI and NEG3. As for NEGl, (as discussed in chapter 3 (3.3.9.1)), it is prefixed to the SM which is at the same time ruled out by the cooccurrence restrictions between SM and INF as discussed in (4.2.1): 159 (11) a. * ti -ku -gul-a NEGl-INF-buy-IND ’not to buy’ b. * ti -ku -fumol -a NEGl-INF-converse-IND ’not to converse’ c. * ti -ku -lind-a NEGl-INF-wait-IND ’not to wait’ However, as noted in chapter 3 (3.3.2), when examples (11.a- c) are regarded as nominal infinitives they are acceptable as shown in (12). (12) a. o -ku -lwan -a ku-abo ti -ku-li-ba-gu(l)-z -a IV-INF-fight-IND 15-their NEGl-lS-NF-Z -buy-CAUS-IND bi-ntu 8 -thing ’their fighting won’t make them buy things’ b. o -ku -boigok-a IV—INF-shout -IND ti -ku-li-ba-fumo(l) -z -a -ge NEGl-15-NF-2 -converce-CAUS-IND-MNR ’shouting won’t make them converce well’ 160 c. o ~ku -lind-a ku-ngi ku-a —mu—laa(l)-z -a IV-INF-wait-IND 15-much 15—IP-3s-sleep -CAUS-IND omwilungu on the way ’his waiting too much has caused him to spend a night on the way (in the wood).’ Note that consonant l is neutralized in favor of _z in the causative form. On the other hand, NEG3 seems as if it co-occurs with an INF morpheme. However, as we discussed in chapter 3 (3.3.9.3), the morpheme ,jgu; which co-occur with NEG3 is what we have referred to as an imaginary marker (UNR). Thus NEG3 and INF do not co-occur as exemplified below: (13) a. ¥ ka -ku -shom -a 'e -bi—o o -ta -bas-e NEG3-INF-study-IND IV 2 -REL Zs NEG2 be able SUBJ * ’do not to study that which you wont be able to understand’ b. 3 ka .-ku -sig -a a -b-ana -b-onka NEG3-INF-leave-IND IV-2-child Z-alone ’do not to leave the children alone’ 161 c. * ka -ku -imuk —a bwanka muno NEG3-INF-wake up-IND morning very ’do not to wake up early (in the) morning’ When in examples (13.a-c), the imaginary marker 3,513”; is substituted for the INF marker, then all the examples are well formed, as shown in (14.a-c). (14) a. ka -ku -shom -a ebyo otabase NEG3-UNR-study IND which you won’t be able ’do not study (things) which you cannot understand’ b. ka -ku -sig -a abana bonka NEG3-UNR-leave-IND children alone ’do not leave the children alone’ 0. ka -ku -imuk -a bwanka muno NEG3-UNR—wake up-IND morning very ’do not to wake up early in the morning’ Examples (13) and (14) therefore, indicate that in Haya you cant get a negative infinitive, instead it would ‘be a negative imperative as in (14). Note that there are phonological realizations that take place in the surface realizations of forms in the examples above as discussed in chapter 3. For example, the vowels i and g are neutralized 162 in favor of the glide 1. Therefore ebig in (14.a) is ...,.... ........ 4.2.3. The Tense Morphemes Tense must co-occur with the SM and IND, except for the near past tense, the remote past and the future remote past ‘which have the past-stem marker —ile: The morphemes -ile and IND may not co-occur: (15) a. ba-lika-gamb—a a -maano. 2 -RFM -tell-IND IV-forbidden ’they will in the far future tell the forbidden’ b. tu-la-mu-tang -ilg ku -zan -a na kato Ip- 3s refuse' INF-play-IND and Kato ’we have refused him to play with Kato.’ c. * mu-a-sig -ile-a busha um 2p- leave -IND empty Example (15.c) is ill formed because the remote past (RP) has a post stem marker igl__e_ and therefore cannot co-occur with IND. Moreover, all tense morphemes co-occur with NEGl and NEG2 as exemplified partially in (16). However, NEG2 cannot follow the infinitive morpheme (as shown in 17), it only co- 163 occurs with only one tense: the present universal habitual (PUH) in a nominal infinitive form as shown in (18): (16) a. ti -zi-l -ile bunyasi bungi NEGl 10-eat-RP grass many ’they (e.g. cows) did not eat a lot of grass’ b. ba-ta —li-gend-a ki—lo 2 -NEGZ—NF-go -IND night ’they should not go at night’ (17) a. * ku—ta -kya-hiig-a ente INF-NEGZ-STL-find-IND cows ’to not still find the cows’ b. * ku-ta -ka -yombek-a enju INF-NEGZ-RMP-build -IND house Examples (17.a, b) show the co-occurrence restrictions of NEGZ with other tenses when. preceded by’ the infinitive (’still’ tense and remote past). As pointed out above NEGZ only cooccurs with the present universal habitual (PUH) as shown below. (18) a. o -ku -ta —ly -a ku-teb -es -a ente IV-INF-NEGZ-eat-IND 15-become thin-CAUS-IND cow ’not eating causes cows to be thin’ 164 b. o -ku -ta —lim -a ku-lum -is -a enjala IV-INF-NEGZ-farm-IND 15-bite-CAUS-IND hunger ’not farming causes hunger’ Furthermore, the tense morphemes cannot cooccur with the imaginary 1kg; except the recent past (19.b). (19) a. t ka -ku -laa ~jum -a a -ba-alimu NEG3-UNR-IF -abuse-IND IV 2 -teacher ’do not abuse the teachers’ b. ba-ku -shuntam-ile ba-ly -a 2 -UNR-sit 2 ~eat-IND ’they could sit and eat’ 4.2.4. The Subject Relative Morpheme The subject relative morpheme takes the form of an initial vowel. The initial vowel (IV) occurs in relative verbs; which always contain a subject morpheme marker. It may not cooccur with COND, NEGl, NEG3, or FOC. Otherwise, it may co-occur with any other prestem, poststem or stem morphemes. Furthermore, the subject relative morpheme may cooccur with any tense. The following are some examples to illustrate these cooccurrence restrictions: 165 (20) a. * e-ki-ka-many-a IV-7-COND-know-IND ’which when know' b. * e-li-ti -shash-a IV 5—NEG1-hurt -IND ’which not hurt’ c. i a -ba-ka -shom-a IV-2 -NEG3—read-IND ’who not read’ d. * e -bi-ni -lum -a IV-8 -FOC-bite-IND Examples (20.a-d) show the co-occurrence restrictions of IV with COND (20.a), NEGl (20.b), NEG3 (20.c) and FOC (20.d). We would attribute the ill-formed form of (20.a) as being a clash of two morpheme the class 12 (diminutive class) and the condition morpheme both of which have the same shape and may be interpreted as occurring in the same slot unless other morphemes entervene. The ill-formed ferm.cfif (20.b) may be explained as due to the position of NEGl, which only appears verb initially, and only negates independent clauses. Dependent clauses like the relative are negated by NEGZ as shown in (21.b) below: (21) a. e -li-shash-a IV 5 -hurt ’which (e.g. eye) hurts’ b. e -li-ta -shash-a IV 5 NEGZ-hurt IND ’which (e.g. eye) do not hurt’ Example (20.0) is ill-formed because NEG3 only cooccurs with the imaginary _-ku- (UNR), or the INF ku- in nominal forms initially. Similarly (21.d) is ill-formed because FOC only appears verb initially and not in any other position. Therefore it cannot cooccur with the IV. (relative) form. This may hypothetically be called ’positional clash.’ Example (20.a), however, may be interpreted as acceptable only when the marker :kg; (COND) is regarded as that of noun class 12, sometimes called the diminutive class (e.g. Doke and Mofokeng for the Sotho language, Byarushengo 1977 for Haya). COND and class 12 share the marker which has the same shape as exemplified below: (22) a -ba-ka-many-a IV-2 -12-know-IND ’who knows it (e.g. a small dog).’ Although we have attributed the ill formed forms to the position they occupy in the verbal paradigm (which is 167 plausible), the major reason is the function these morphemes perform in the verb. As discussed in detail in the next section, IV. (subject relative morpheme), NEGI, NEG3 and FOC introduce dependent clauses, such that the cooccurrence of any two of these morphemes are restricted because of what we might call "identical functional clash." Furthermore, IV. is followed by SM, but not vice versa, a point we have discussed at length when discussing the linear order in chapter 3. (23) a. e -zi-li-gul-w -a IV 10-NF-buy-Pass-IND ’which (e.g. cows) will be bought’ b. * zi-e -li—gul-w -a 10-IV-NF-buy-PASS-IND 4.2.5. The Conditional Morpheme The COND morpheme may not co—occur with IV, NEGl, NEG3 or FOC; it may co-occur with any tense except the remote past (RMP) and the remote future (RMF). (24) a. 3 a -ba-ka —li-shutul-a IV 2 COND NF carry IND ’who if will carry’ b. (25) a. b. (26) a. b. (27) a. N' 96 168 ka -a -ba-li—shutul-a COND-IV-Z -NF-carry -IND ka -ti -ba—shek —a COND-NEGl-Z -laugh-IND ’if not they laugh’ ti -ka -ba-shek -a NEGl-COND-Z -laugh-IND ka —ni -ba-many-a COND—FOC-Z -know—IND ’if they’ ni -ba—ka -many-a FOC-2 -COND know-IND ’they (indeed) if know’ ka -ka -ku -zin -a COND-NEG3-UNR-sing-IND ’if not sing’ ka -ku -ka -zin -a NEGS-UNR-COND-sing-IND ’not if sing’ 169 In examples (24-27) various cooccurrence restrictions of COND with other morphemes are shown in pairs following and preceding the COND. The cooccurrence restrictions of COND with IV (example 24), NEGl (example 25) and FOC (example 26) may be explained again as ’similar functional clash.’ All the three morphemes introduce the dependent clause and therefore they cannot cooccur. Similarly, although NEGB does not occur in the same position as the other morphemes (IV, NEG, and FOC), its function is similar to them, as exemplified below: (28) ka -ku -ly -a o -ta -nab -ile mi-kono NEG3-UNR-eat-IND Zs-NEGZ-wash-RP 4 -hand ’do not eat without washing hands (your)’ Moreover, COND does not cooccur with the remote past (RMP) and remote future (RMF) tenses. (29) * ka -ba-ka -it -a COND-2 -RMP-kill-IND Example (29) is ill-formed because COND does not co-occur with (RMP) tense. The RMP 35;; has the same shape as the OM (kg) which agrees with class 12. Thus example (29) may be regarded as well formed when the kg, of the RMP tense is substituted for the ka of OM (class 12) as illustrated: tut-m 170 (30) ka-ba-ka-it-a COND-Z-IZ-kill-IND ’when they kill it (e.g. a small goat).’ The same reasons apply in the co-occurrence restrictions of COND and the remote future (RMF) where the marker -lika— clashes with the marker _;l_i__; of near past tense and the marker :kg; clashes with the 3kg; of the OM (class 12) (see also chapter 5 (5.3.3.2)). (31) * ka -ba-lika-gul-a COND-2 -RMF -buy-IND ’? if they (in the remote future) buy’ However, example (31) is regarded as well formed when the marker -lika- is split into two markers: -li- with the role of the near future tense marker (NF) and :5a; with the role of. the OM (class 12), as discussed in chapter 3 (3.3.5.3.3.). (32) ka -ba-li-ka-gul-a COND 2 -NF-12-buy-IND ’when they will buy it’ 4.2.6. The Focus Morpheme FOC occurs only in the present universal habitual (PUH) and the "still" tense (-kya—). It may only co-occur with 171 the SM, the IND and the SUBJ and the ’still’ (-kya-) tense morphemes as exemplified: (33) a. ni -ba-kya—lil-a FOC-Z -STL-cry-IND ’they are still crying’ b. ni -ba-lil-e FOC-2 -cry-SUBJ ’let them cry’ (34) a. * ni -mu-laa-lil-a FOC-2p-IF -cry-IND b. * ni -ba—a-lil-ag-a FOC-2- -cry- -IND Examples (34.a) and (34.b) indicate that the FOC morpheme marker may not cooccur with the immediate future morpheme nor with the immediate past habitual morpheme. . These are examples of certain tenses which do not cooccur with FOC; otherwise all except the (PUH) and STL do not cooccur. The FOC morpheme also does not co-occur with any NEG morpheme, IV, or COND. We will only show NEG morpheme examples here. Other examples have been covered in the previous sections. 172 (35) a. * ni -tu-ta -jum -a FOC-Ip-NEGZ-abuse-IND b. * ni -ka -ku -log —a FOC-NEGB-UNR-witch-IND c. * ti -ni -ba-hend -a NEGl-FOC-2 -break-IND Again (35.b) can only be interpreted as well formed when the NEG3 marker is regarded as the SM (class 12) and the -ku- of UNR is realized as the OM. (36) ni -ka-ku-log -a FOC-12-2s-witch-IND ’it is bewitching you’ Example (35.a) whose affirmative form is: (37) ni -tu-jum -a FOC-ls-abuse-IND ’we are abusing’ can only be negated by deleting the FOC marker and prefixing the NEGl to the SM as indicated below: 173 (38) ti--tu—li-ku-jum-a NEGl-Ip-NF—INF-abuse-IND ’we are not abusing’ This restriction is also observed in Kikuyu On the other hand Tharaka permits F00 and (Perez (Harford) 1987). 4.2.7. The Negative Morphemes NEGl may co-occur with any tense ,therefore, (cf. discussion of tense markers (Bergvall 1987). NEG to co-occur of the verb, for more detail) since it only negates main verbs (cf. chapter 3), it may not cooccur ‘with IV, COND, or FOC as we mentioned in the previous sections. (39) a. e -bi-kol-w -a IVé8 -do -PASS-IND ’which (things) are done’ b. ti -e -bi-kol-w -a NEGl-IV-8 -do -PASS-IND (40) a. ka -ba-laa-kul -a COND-2 -IF -grow-IND ’when they grow’ 174 b. * ti -ka -ba-laa-kul -a NEGl-COND-Z -IF -grow-IND ’not when they grow’ (41) a. ni -tu—shom -a FOC-Ip-study-IND ’we are studying’ b. * ti-ni-tu-shom-a NEGl-FOC-Ip—study-IND ’not we are studying’ In examples (39) to (41) the second pair of each example is ill-formed» illustrating the cooccurrence):restrictions of NEGl with IV, COND and FOC, respectively. These morphemes introduce dependent clauses, so that they cannbt co-occur with NEGl, which, as stated above, negates only main verbs (independent clauses). , NEG2 co-occurs with the SM, all tenses except remote future and the infinitive morpheme. When NEG2 follows the infinitive morpheme it only' cooccur with the present universal habitual (PUH) as discussed above in section (4.1.3). NEGZ may cooccur with (IV), and (COND) only when following them. 175 (42) ba-ta —li-galuk -a hanu lundi 2 -NEGZ IF-return-IND here again ’they should not return here again’ (43) a. ku -ta -mu-jun -a kiki INF NEG2 3s-help-IND why ’what is restricting you from helping him’ b. * ku —ta -lika-mu-jun —a kiki INF-NEGZ RMF 3s help—IND why NEG3 only co~occurs with the ’imaginary’ morpheme marker (see 4.2.2 above). (44) ka -ku —it -a amani gawe busha NEG3-UNR-’kill’-IND energy yours for nothing ’don’t waste your time’ (lit. don’t kill your energy for nothing). Finally, the NEG morphemes may not co-occur with each other as exemplified in (45.a-c). (45) a. 3 ti -ba-ta -1i -shom-a NEGI-SM-NEGZ-TNS-read-IND b. * ba-ta -ka -ku -songok -a SM-NEGZ-NEG3-INF-descend-IND 176 c. X ti —ya-ta -ka -ku -yet -a NEGl SM-NEGZ-NEG3-INF-call-IND The three examples in (45.a-c) indicate that Haya does not allow "double" or "triple" negatives in a verbal construction. This is semantically plausible because Haya verbs usually carry one assertion and this on; assertion is negated by gng negative morpheme. Thus far, the co-occurrence restrictions of the prestem morphemes have been discussed. These restrictions are . summarized in a table form as in table 12 at the end of the chapter on (page 214). 4.3. Co—occurrence Restrictions of Poststem Morphemes: 4.3.1. The Indicative Morpheme The only significant cooccurrence restrictions in this group hold for IND and SUBJ, which may not cooccur with each other. IND must occur with all tenses, in infinitives and certain imperatives. (46) a. yanguh-a ’hurry-IND’ ’hurry’ 177 b. * yanguh-a-e hurry-IND-SUBJ c. * yanguh—e-a hurry-SUBJ-IND As illustrated in examples (46.b-c), the IND morpheme cannot cooccur with the SUBJ mor‘pheme whether the SUBJ morpheme follows (46.b) or precedes the IND (46.c0. The indicative morpheme is obligatory, used when no other is called for. However, the subjunctive and .:..i.....1._. byashaalila 8 -IP -ROOT -IND ’they (e.g. cassava) have become bitter’ (c) that the internal structure respects a specific order of morphemes involved. For example (110) ba-laa-lu -ba-ching-ul-il -a 2 -IF -IONH-2H close-RV-APL-IND ’they will open (unclose) it for them (e.g. door)’ is a possible word but (111) X a ~laa-ba-ching -ba -ul -lu -il- IND-IF-Z-close-ZH-RV-IONH-APL is not possible. Furthermore, the discussion made in this chapter strongly supports the hypothesis by morphologists such as Aronoff (1981), Scalise (1986), Bybee (1985) and others, that morphology interacts with other parts of grammar, namely: syntax, semantics, phonology (as shown in chapter 2) to mention a few. In the next chapter (chapter 5) we take up one such theory, by B (1985) whose theory is semantically 212 based and apply her theory to the linear order of the Haya verbal morphemes. 213 CHAPTER 4 ENDNOTES 1 Khamisi (1983:94) calls what we have named stem morphemes as syntactic lexical suffixes. 3 Fossilized intransitive is a term we have borrowed from Khamisi (ibid:103). 3 Eggggk;angan; ’bend one another’ sounds a little odd because usually, for bending human beings another lexical item is preferred, which is: inam-y -angan-a bend-CAUS-RCP -IND ’cause one another to bend’ where there is a causative morpheme involved. 21111 Table 12. Sueeary of Cooccurrence Restrictions of Prestee and Poststee Horpheees (IV) (COND) (NEGI) (FOC) (SH) (NEGZ) (TNS) (MEGS (INF (IND)/(SU8J) UNR (IV) ‘ - - + + - - + + (COND) - - - + + t - - f - (NEGI) ‘ - - + - + - - + - (FOC) ’ - - - + - t - - + / + (Sh) + + + + + + - - + / + (N662) + + - - ' + 1 - + - - (TNS) t 1 + g + t - - 1 - (Recs - - - - - - - . , - (INF/UNR) - - - - - - - + + - (IND) + + + + + + + + i + - (SUBJ) + + - + + + + + + - 1. As for tense (j) leans that only certain tenses eay cooccur with that particular eorpheee. Refer to specifics in the text. 2. +/+ eeans either indicative or subjunctive eay be applied 215 Table 13. Cooccurrence Restrictions of the Stem Morphemes. OM RFM CAUS APL PASS RCP ST RPT OM + — + + - - - + RFM - + + - - - + CAUS + + — +’ + - + APL + + - + + + - + PASS - - + + - L - RCP + + + + - - + ST - - — - - - - RPT + + + + - + - 1. The ’—’ marker indicate the cooccurrence restriction of the morpheme involved. 2. ’+’ marker indicate the possibility of cooccurrence. CHAPTER 5 PRINCIPLES OF VERBAL MORPHOLOGY 5.1. Introduction In this chapter we lay out the essential points of B’s theory tn) account for' the recurrent. properties of morphological systems including fusion and allomorphy.1 We also discuss the application of the Haya data to B’s theory. The chapter is presented as follows: in the second section we discuss B’s theory, followed by a discussion of why we chose Haya as a target of study in section three. In section four we discuss the application of B’s theory to Haya. Note that we do not intend to discuss all of the verbal morphemes, but will discuss a few of them and will make some general observations about linear order. Two sets of morphemes are discussed at length; the subject, and object morphemes. We conclude this chapter in section five by commenting on a theory of Paradigm Economy presented by Carstairs (1983) and its relevance to the present study, followed by the summary and conclusion of this chapter. 5.2. B’s theory The major assumption behind B’s theory is that the formal properties of morphological expressions correlate 216 217 directly with aspects of the meaning expressed; the relation between meaning and form is not entirely arbitrary (4) (Note that numbers in brackets are page numbers). By "form," B refers specifically to the ways in which meaning elements may be related to each other formally (11). She gives three such ways:2 1. Two meaning elements may be combined in a single nondecomposable lexical item, which she refers to as a "lggicalngxprgssign. An example is the English verb "kill," which combines the meaning elements "die" and "cause" (11). 2. Two meaning elements may be seen to be expressed by separate units, but these units are combined into a single words B refers to a combination such as this as an ”inflggtignaLwexprgsgigg." An example is the English word "walked," which combines the meaning element "go on foot by taking steps" (13), which is expressed by the unit "walk" and the meaning element "past tense,’ which is expressed by the unit "—ed" (11). 3. Two meaning elements are expressed by separate words, referred to by B as a mtagtigwexpressign." An example is the English expression "come to know," which combines syntactically the meaning elements of "inchoative" and "know" (11). B points out fthat the same meaning elements also find lexical expression as the verb "realize" (12). B proposes two principles predicting what kind of formal relation will hold between two meaning elements and, 218 in particular, which meaning elements are most likely to be expressed as inflectional categories: i.e., falling under (2) above. These principles are called Relevance and Generality. B defines Relevance as follows: ”A meaning element is relevant to another meaning element if the. semantic content of the first directly affects or modifies the semantic content of the second" (13; italics omitted). B makes the prediction that two meaning elements which are relevant to each other may find expression lexically or inflectionally, whereas elements which are irrelevant to each other will only combine syntactically. B gives an example of the predictive power of Relevance from lexicalization in English (13). There is an English verb walk, which encodes the semantic notion "to go on foot by ‘taking steps." She considers the Relevance of two additional semantic notions to this one: that of doing something "through water" and that of doing something ”on a cloudy day." According to her intuitions, "through water" is highly relevant to "go on foot by taking steps," and hence we find the two combined in the English verb wade. ‘Cn the other hand, "on, a. cloudy day" is not particularly relevant to "go on foot by taking steps,’ and hence we find no verb which means "go on foot on a cloudy day" (14). B defines Generality as follows: "(For) an inflectional category (to be general, it) must be applicable to‘all stems of the appropriate semantic and syntactic category and must obligatorily' occur' in the .appropriate syntactic context" 219 (17; material in parentheses added). She then suggests that a morphological process must have relatively low semantic content to be general. As an example of high semantic content restricting Generality, she cites motion verbs in Latin, which take a number of prefixes indicating direction: Since these prefixes have high semantic content, they are restricted to a subclass of verbs, namely motion verbs, and consequently lack Generality, since they are not applicable to all verbs. These prefixes therefore cannot be considered to be an inflectional category (17).3 From: these two principles, B make a number of predictions which are now discussed next. 5.2.1. Predictions arising from Relevance and Generality Before we discuss the predictions, note that based on a survey of 50 of the world’s languages, B came up with a list of categories which she regards as most likely to be inflectional, which we reproduce below. without details or examples (20). We will give and discuss examples when we discuss the application of the Haya data; These categories are: (a) Valgngg refers to differences in number or roles of arguments that the verb stem can take. (B:20) Examples are tritransitive (four arguments), ditransitive (three arguments), transitive (two arguments) and intransitive (one argument), and causative forms for ‘which the number 'of arguments varies. 220 (b) Vgige. distinctions change the relation that the surface subject bears to *the verb (B:20, citing Barber 1975). Examples include active, passive, reflexive, reciprocal. (c) Aspggt, refers to the ‘way iJi which the internal temporal constituency of the situation is viewed. Examples include perfective and imperfect. Imperfective verb forms indicate that the action has not been completed while perfective verb forms indicate the completion of the action. (d) Lens; locates the situation with respect to an established point in time, either the moment of speech, or some other point in time (B:22). (e) Mpgd refers to the way the speaker presents the truth of the proposition in the context of discourse and the real world, including probability, possibility and certainty (22) B points out that this category includes, but is not limited to, expressions of assertion (indicative), non- assertion. (subjunctive), command (imperatived and. warning (admonitive). (f) Agreemen; as a verbal inflectional category formally encodes the relationship between a verb and its arguments in terms of ‘features such as person, number, gender' and. other' characteristics of participants in the situation. These categories will be mentioned frequently in the discussion of B’s theory and its application to Haya. We next turn to the predictions. 221 5.2.1.1. Relevance The principle of Relevance makes three major predictions: order, frequency, and fusion. of the verbal forms to the stem, the details of which we will get into momentarily. At the moment it is in order to mention that B discussed other independent predictions which we were unable to apply to Haya.‘ Moreover, it is appropriate to mention here that this study will test the predictions which B draws from her principle, but will not evaluate the degree to which the predictions actually follow from the principles. 5.2.1.1.1. Order This prediction, which will now be referred to as linear order, has to do with the order of categories with respect to a stem. B claims that the categories that are more relevant to the verb stem will occur closer to the stem than those that are less relevant (24). By this she means a greater effect on the semantic content of the stem. Conversely, the less releVant the category is to the stem, the further away it is located from the verb stem. B argues that this principle applies because~ it affects elements before they are bound and are still movable in the clause. This argument implies that the more a concept has to do with the content of the verb, the closer it will occur to the verb stem (211). Therefore when these elements become bound (affixes) to the verb stem, their order will be the same. 222 That is, the more relevant affixes are closer to the verb- stem, and therefore the less relevant affixes are further away from the stem.5 B’s argument can be illustrated using the following schema: (1) x-y—z-stem-a-b—c According to B’s hypothesis, relevance increases from x to g and from g to a. More specifically B proposes that the following inflectional categories common on verbs (20ff) may be arranged on the following scale of relevance (24) where (+) indicates the most relevant end of. the scale, and (-) the least relevant. (2) Valence-V0ice-Aspect-Tense-Mood-Agreement m --------------- ‘ -------------------- (-) Let us assume that the verb stem may be arranged in two ways: (a) left of the stem and (b) right of the stem as in (3.a) and (3.6) (using B’s categories). (3) a. AGREEMENT-MOOD-TENSE-ASPECT-VOICE-VALENCE-VERB STEM (3) b. VERB-VALENCE-VOICE-ASPECT-TENSE-MOOD-AGREEMENT STEM 223 As shown in (3) B’s theory predicts a mirror-image order of morphemes on the left of the verb-stem and on the right of the verb-stem, given that "the most relevant inflectional categories occur closest to the verb-stem, and the least relevant occur at the greatest distance from the verb—stem.” An important point which B makes is that these categories form. a continuum. The continuum hypotheses is a very important point, for unlike the traditional approach (Chomsky and Halle 1968, Siegel 1974, Aronoff 1976 . . .1 B does not make a sharp division between categories that fall under derivational or inflectional categories as has been the case within the traditional approach but they are relevant to the concepts of Relevance and Generality, as will be discussed shortly. Thus, B’s approach may be summarized as follows: (1) the more relevant the categories are, the more interior they appear with respect to the verb stem. (2) the less relevant the categories are, the more exterior they appear with respect to the verb stem. Let us now discuss the second prediction: frequency. 5.2.1.1.2. Frequency B tested the frequency of morphological categories for verbs in 50 languages. She made two predictions: that there were (1) few inflections among the most highly relevant categories (29) and (2) more inflections among the least relevant categories. These statements can be schematized as 224 in (4) where 1+) refers to a more of a category and (-) less of a category and INFL means inflectional and RLV means relevance. (4) a. [~INFL]-[+RLV] b. [+INFL]-[-RLV] However, frequency predictions can only be tested comparatively across languages. Nevertheless, as noted above, Haya has all of the verbal inflectional categories (see chapter 3). Haya, canr be added as another sample language to B’s data. 5.2.1.1.3. Degree of fusion B’s third prediction concerns fusion. She predicts that the more relevant the morphemes are to the stem, the more likely there is to be fusion; the less relevant the morphemes the less likely there is to be fusion. She claims there are two reasons for this: (1) that relevant categories occur closer to the verb in the syntactic string (2) the psychological restructuring of two words into one depends on the relatedness of the semantic elements being joined, and their ability to form a coherent semantic whole (38). These predictions are hierarchically represented in this order. ( 5 ) Lexical-derivational-inflectional-free graeeatical-syntaotic (+) < H 225 Again, B states that these expressions form a continuum that ranges ”from the most highly fused means of expression, lexical. expression, to the most. loosely joined. means of expression, syntactic or periphrastic expression” (12). In (51 the (+1 marker indicates the most highly fused and (-1 indicates the most loosely joined. Again, in order to have a clear discussion of these categories their meanings are briefly given without getting into details. (a) lexigal egpressigg. In a lexical expression, two or more semantic elements are expressed :Ui a. monomorphemic lexical item. B mentions some examples from various languages including English. In English as mentioned earlier, the lexical items: (6) a. kill combines the semantic elements ’die’ and ’cause’ (to die). b. drop combines the semantic elements ’fall’ and ’cause’ (to fall). (b) Der'vatiggul Exprmmsigg. B points out that a "derivational expression resembles lexical expression in that derivational morphemes are often restricted in applicability and idiosyncratic in formation or meaning. It resembles inflectional expression in that two distinct morphemes are combined in a single word" (B:12). Three of 226 B’s verbal categories falls under derivational expression. These categories are valence, voice and aspect (99). As an example, B discusses the category of aspegt as a lexical, derivational and inflectional category (100). Let us review what she says about the aspectual derivational categories. "Inceptive and iterative tend to be restrictedto stative verbs, so in languages with a morphological inceptive (or inchoative) marker, this marker is derivational.” She also points out that iterative markers are derivational because they are applicable only to some verbs which describe events that take place in a very short period of time, such as ggwgh, and .blink, but not. to stative verbs (100). The example given is the morpheme wade. in the Kiwai language (Ray 1933) which indicates "the repetition of an action or its frequent or regular performance"; such a morpheme resembles the Haya repetitive (RPT) morpheme discussed in chapter 3 (3.5.51. (c1 IatLeetiQrabexnrelssaen . In an inflectional expression, each semantic element is expressed in an individual unit, but these units are bound into a single word. Examples are affixes added to a stem or a change of stem, such as the English regular past tense, walkgd vs irregular bggwght. Inflectional morphemes obligatorily accompany the verb-stem. They are generally unrestricted in applicability. (d) Free grammaticgl. Free grammatical morphemes belong to a closed class and occur in a fixed position. 227 They "resemble inflections in that they make up contrast sets that are obligatory in certain environments and they have positional restrictions” (12). B adds that they "resemble periphrastic expressions in that they are not bgwnd to lexical stems." Unfortunately B does not give an example of these morphemes, but simply states that they are not studied in her work, "although their existence is recognized by the theory to be proposed" (12). (e) Syntactic expression. By syntactic expression B no... no. means that semantic elements are expressed in separate words . For example ’ come to know ’ contains the semantic notions ’ inchoative ’ and ’ know . ’ These notions are also expressed by a single word ’realize.’ The syntactic expression is sometimes called a "periphrastic" expression (ibid:12). We now turn to the second principle Generality and discuss how the predictions which follow from this principle. 5.2.1.2. Generality According to the determinant of generality "an inflectional category must be applicable to all stems of the appropriate semantic and syntactic category and must obligatorily occur in the appropriate syntactic context” (17). Simply stated, generality "involves the degree to which a particular process is applicable within a particular domain" (Walker 1986:4941. 228 As was the case with Relevance, Generality is proposed to express the same three major predictions: order, frequency and fusion. These predictions are the reverse of those predicted by the Relevance prin‘ciple. We briefly discuss these predictions based on Generality. 5.2.1.2.1. Order This prediction concerns the proximity of verbal categories to the verb-stem. Unlike Relevance, the most general categories occur at the greatest distance from the verb-stem. Using the linear order of the verbal morphemes as proposed by B, (7) VERB STEM-VALENCE-VOICE-ASPECT-TENSE-MOOD-AGREEMENT 1-1 ----------------------------------- (+1 Generality increases from valence to agreement, where ((+1 indicates more general and (-1 less general. In other words agreement is more general to the verb-stem than valence. As also was the case with Relevance, Generality forms a continuum from the less general to the more general categories . In this case, valence is less general than voice, voice is less general than aspect . . . and mood is less general than agreement. This ordering may be represented as in (8) where ) indicates is more general than 229 (8) AGREEMENT>MOOD>TENSE>ASPECT>VOICE>VALENCE-VERB STEM B’s argument may be summarized as follows: the more general the category is the more exterior it appears with respect to the verb—stem and conversely the less general the category is the more interior it is located with respect to the verb stem. This generalization is the reverse of that of the Relevance principle with respect to linear order.6 Let us now discuss the second prediction in relation to Generality, which is frequency. 5.2.1.2.2. Frequency B’s test of 50 languages indicates that there are larger .numbers of inflections among the least relevant categories than there are among the most highly relevant categories. In Haya (see chapter 3) there are thirteen least relevant categories and ten highly relevant categories, which support B’s prediction.7 Again, as earlier mentioned, -frequency and order coincide. The further away the categories are from the verb-stem, the more likely they are to be highly General and the higher the frequency. The closer the categories are to the verb-stem they more likely they are least General and the lower the frequency. Thus, the frequency prediction is summarized as‘ follows: (a) there are many inflectional categories among the most highly General categories and conversely (b) there are few inflectional categories among the least General 230 categories. These statements can be schematized as in (9) where (+1 indicates more of a category and (-) indicates less of a category and GEN means General, and INF means Inflectional. (9) a. [+GEN) - [+INFL] b. {-GEN] — l-INFL] Now let us move on to the third prediction, which is the degree of fusion. 5.2.1.2.3 Degree of Fusion Again, as was the case with frequency, the degree of fusion also coincides with the order of categories. B, as mentioned earlier, predicts that the more relevant the morphemes are to the verb-stem the closer they are to the verb-stem and the more likely it is that fusion will take place. Conversely, therefore, the less relevant‘ the morphemes are to the verb-stem the further away they are to the verb-stem and the less likely there is to be fusion. As discussed above, it is predicted that Relevant categories are more likely to fuse than General categories. i As was the case with other predictions, B represents the fusion hierarchy as follows: ( 1(1) Lexical-derivational-inflectionaI-free graeeatical-syntactic. 231 A syntactic expression is the most loosely joined expression and therefore more General, while the lexical expression is the most highly fused expression and therefore less General (more relevant). These predictions can be represented in a continuum hierarchy as shown in (11) where > indicates more General than and (+1 a more fused category and (-1 a less fused category. ( 1 1 ) syntactic > free graeeatical > inflectional > derivational > leXical (fl ‘ (fl 5.2.2. Synthesis The discussion thus far has shown the intertwining of B’s two major principles, Relevance and Generality, and the predictions that are derived from these principles, which are: order, frequency and fusion. The two principles and their predictions coincide in the sense that while Relevance predicts closeness of the category or categories to the stem, fewer categories which are closer to the stem, and more likelihood of categories which are closer to the stem to fuse, Generality predicts the opposite of Relevance. It predicts that categories which are General are farther away from the stem, that more of these categories are inflectional and that they are less likely to fuse. This analysis may be schematized into a redundancy feature matrix as suggested in (12.a) and (12.b) 232 (12) a. + CLOSE TO STEM - INFL ---- [RELEVANCE] + FUSION - CLOSE TO STEM INFL ---- [GENERALITY] - FUSION + 5.2.3. Comments on B’s theory It is not the purpose of this dissertation to review B’s work (see Aronoff 1987, and Walker 1986). However, a few comments are in order here. B mentions three predictions based on the two principles Relevance and Generality: Order, frequency, fusion. Of the three predictions, order received. more emphasis throughout her work than any of the others. From this order, the degree of fusion is also predictable - with those categories near the stem most likely to fuse with it than those further away from the stem. While such a claim might be appropriate with fusional languages, it might be a problem with agglutinative languages like Haya. Also, the hierarchy of order of inflectional categories has been questioned. For example, Aronoff (1987) questions "why should voice, which relates argument structure to syntactic subject, be more relevant than aspect, which is confined semantically to the verb itself? (118). We shall not attempt to answer this sort of question; but rather, attempt to apply B’s theory as she 233 gives it to the Haya data. Interestingly, this application sheds light on the analysis of two problematic morphological categories, inflectional and derivational (see her chapter 4: 81-110). In her work B reviewed a number of linguistic definitions offered thus far (Greenberg 1954, Mathews 1974, Anderson 1982, Kurytowicz 1964, Bloomfield 1933, Nida 1946 . . . ) and pointed out some major problems, with each of them (which are not of immediate relevance here). She then proposed to treating Inflection and derivation as single category constituting a continuum from inflection~ to derivation. Such an approach, she claims fits her general principles of Relevance and Generality as has been discussed above. This is a claim that forms the discussion of the applicability of Haya data to her theory in the next section. But before that it is pointed out why Haya was chosen as target of study. 5.3. Haya as target of study As seen in the previous section, B’s theory is intended to cover a wide, if not the entire, range of morphological phenomena found in human language and has been tested on a diverse sample of 50 languages. However, it is by no means obvious that .B’s specific predictions necessarily follow from her very generally formulated principles, given the potential for wide interpretation of such terms as 7directly affects" or "modifies" in the definition of Relevance. 234 Aronoff (1987:117) notes the problem of circularity of B’s definitions. For example, he questions how we know what is relevant. Even the justification provided by B for relative ranking of individual cases leaves a lot to be desired. Such cases as ’walking through water’ is lexicalized as wgdg because ’whether one has one’s feet on dry land in water is quite relevant to the act of walking’ (13), however, does not seem to suggest that B has developed any specifically or generally applicable criteria for Relevance. We will apply the predictions B makes concerning the ordering of specific categories to some of the data we have assembled on Haya verbal morphology in chapters 3 and 4, and confine our attention to those morphological issues that this theory touches_upon. The discussion will focus on inflectional expression to the exclusion of the other two types of formal expression, lexical expression and syntactic expression, because this work has focussed on the verbal morphemes of Haya, which are inflectional according to 3’3 definitions. Also, the discussion will concentrate on linear order, with a few comments on frequency and no mention of fusion. This approach may be justified by first considering the position of Haya within a morphological typology of the world’s languages. 235 5.3.1. Typological position of Haya Languages are classified into structural types, three of which are relatively common: isolating, agglutinating and fusional (Lyons 1968, Comrie 1981, Bybee 19851.3 An isolating language is defined as "one in which all words are invariable" (Lyons (1968:1891). This means that all words are without internal morphological structure. Examples of such languages include Chinese and Vietnamese, as can be illustrated below in a Vietnamese sentence (Comrie 1981:40). (14) Khi toi den nha ban toi, chung toi bat when I come house friend I PLURAL I begin dau lam bai. do lesson. ’When I came to my friends’ house, we began to ' do lessons.’ This sentence shows that each "of the words in this sentence is invariable, there being no morphological variation. . ." (ibid:40). For example the word gig}; indicates both the tense ’come/came,’ and bat dgw indicate the tense begin/began. This language also has to; for both "I" and "my,' and ”plurality is indicated, in the case of pronouns, by the addition of a separate word rather than by morphological means, so that the plural of toi "I" is chung toi ’we’" (ibid:40). 236 Agglutinating languages are languages in which words are composed of a sequence of morphemes with each morpheme representing more or less one strand of meaning. B cites Care as an example of an agglutinating language; and gives the following sentence as an example (1985:451.9 (14) Garo /sok -ba -ku -ja -ma/=/sokbakujama/ arrive-toward-yet-neg-int ’has he not yet arrived?’ Example (141 shows that the word jsokbakujama/ is made of morphemes each with a particular meaning. Haya is also a highly agglutinating language, the details of which we will come to momentarily. Fusional languages (sometimes called inflecting e.g. Lyons 1977:72) display no clear cut boundary between morphemes. Expression of (different categories within the same word is fused together. That is unlike AGGLUTINATIVE languages, there is 1K) one-to-one~ correspondence between these morphemes and the linear sequence of MORPHS (Comrie 1981). In other words, a single morpheme typically represents several strands of meaning. Examples of fusional languages include Latin, Greek, Hebrew and Arabic, to mention a few. The following is an example from classical Latin (Bybee 1985) and Hebrew, respectively (ibid:45). (15) Latin: amo ’I love’ 237 The morpheme —0 simultaneously represents the following: 1 present tense ii active voice iii lst person singular agreement iv indicative mood. (16) Hebrew (the verb ’to write’) Past 33 masc: katav Present, 33 masc: kotev Future, 33 masc: yixtov Imperative, masc. sg: ktov "the consonants of the stem interdigitate with the vowels that signal the inflectional categories, so that IE) clear segmentation is possible" (45). The three terms ’isolating,’ ’agglutinating’ and ’fusional’ refer to idealized language types. In reality, languages can rarely be characterized as being exclusively of one of these types. For example, Haya, as well as other Bantu languages is highly agglutinating: (17) Haya ba-laa-mu-jun -a SM-TNS-OM-STEM-IND ’they will help him’ 238 Example (17) shows that, the morphemes'occur sequentially, one per syllable. However, it also shows a sign of fusion: (18) tu-ka -mu-taigy -a SM-TNS-DO-say good bye-IND ’we said good-bye to him/her’ the morpheme tu- ’we’ simultaneously represents (19) i gender - first person ii number - plural iii agreement - agrees with noun class 2 abantu "people" And Haya also shows signs of isolation, such as in the following morphemes: kygnkg ’but,’ Qlwekyg ’therefore,’ . u" ’daily’ as P. 1 1 contextualized in (20.a-c). (20) a. Nyakato ya-a -chumb-a kxgwkg . Nyakato SM-TNS cook -IND but ti -ya-a -yogy -a byombo NEGl-SM-TNS-wash -IND dishes ’Nyakato cooked but she didn’t wash the dishes’ 239 b. Kamuzora a -la-yombek-ile glwgkyg na iwe Kamuzora SM- -build - ~therefore and o —yombek-e you farm ~SUBJ ’Kamuzora has already built (a house), you should also build (a house).’ c. Nyamwiza a -shom-a bwli Nyamwiza SM-read-IND daily ’Nyamwiza reads daily’ The words kmnka ’ but , ’ olwekyo ’ therefore , ’ 121-1.1.11. ’ daily ’ have no internal morphological structure, unlike many other words in Haya as shown in the above sentences . But in general Haya shows overwhelmingly agglutinative morphology. 5.3.2. Appropriateness of Haya as test of B’s theory The highly agglutinating nature of Haya makes it particularly appropriate to evaluating a theory concerning the linear order of inflectional categories: a. First of all, Haya has a lot of inflectional categories: most, if not 'all of the categories on B’s hierarchy are expressed inflectionally in Haya as verbal affixes. In addition, there are also verbal inflections which do not fit any of her categories particularly well. Before we get into the details of these discussions, it is in order here to mention that, in evaluating B’s predictions 240 using the Haya data, a number of problems were encountered, similar to those encountered by B in her analysis. Three problems are discussed here: (1) determining relative order, (2) distinguishing in the light of current knowledge, categories like Tense and Aspect, and (3) identifying Haya morphemes which represent B’s categories. Concerning Relative Order, there are a number of categories which cannot cooccur in a verb and which occur in the same position, for example, the focus marker (FOC), the initial vowel (IV), which signifies the relative morpheme, the conditional morpheme (COND) and the negative morpheme (NEGl). We have represented such mutually exclusive categories with bracket notation: (21) FOC IV COND NEG (cf. chapter 3:53). Second is the problem of indeterminacy of categories such as tense and aspect. In Haya as far as we know, there has been relatively little work on tense and aspect and as of now it is impossible to state for sure whether a particular morpheme belongs to the category TNS or ASP. The relevant morphemes have therefore been categorized as occurring in a single TNS/ASP slot (27). Third, is the problem that, certain Haya morphemes do not readily fit B’s categories.1° These morphemes occupy the slots in the model (2211abelled xl-xa. 241 Thus far the Haya model can be represented as: STEM (22) Xl-AGR-TNS/ASP-Xz RFM - ROOT - X3 OM ExtensionsilNDi}LOC-MNR—Q-INT SUB X1 slot includes the mutually exclusive morphemes FOC , IV COND NEG and X2 is an ‘{INF}_ morpheme. UNR As we have mentioned in chapter 3 section ~(3.21 the root occurs in the middle of the verb so that morphemes that are more relevant appear closer on both sides of the root as shown in (22). Again looking at the Haya data, B’s predictions are borne out, the most relevant categories, are closer to the root on both sides, and the least relevant morphemes are further away from the root, which indicates they' are most general. This order of the Haya ‘verbal morphemes based on the order of morphemes predicted by B raises a number of questions: (1) do all the morphemes as they appear in the Haya verb follow B’s predictions? (2) if not how do we modify B’s theory or account for the ’misfit’ of these morphemes with respect to B’ 3 ‘model . These are some of the questions we raised in chapter 1 ( 1 . 1'1 . An attempt to answer these questions, leads us into the theoretical part of this study. We will shortly discuss the status of the object. morpheme ‘marker in respect to 18’s linear order prediction in (5.3.3.4) below. 242 b. Secondly, the more agglutinating and less fusional nature of the inflectional morphemes of Haya make linear order much easier to evaluate. What fusion there is (of. chapter 2) does not obscure the relative order of the morphemes which undergo it. c. Thirdly, we would also like to point out that B’s original 50 language survey did not include any Bantu languages, although at one point in the text she discusses causativization in Luganda (18). 5.3.3. Application of the theory to Haya We begin the discussion with a classification of the Haya verbal morphemes (apart from the SM and OM, which are discussed in the next section) according to B’s verbal inflectional categories, with comments on the extent to which their position in the Haya verb matches B’s predictions. 5.3.3.1. The Pres-stem Morphemes (non—stem) a. Initial Vowel As we discussed in section (3.3.6) and (4.2.4) IV is prefixed to the subject marker to form the subject relative morpheme. This kind of morpheme is not discussed in B’s work. Maybe B would have categorized it under agreement, if anything, because its form varies according to the noun class of the subject of its verb (see table 4). If so, the 243 IV occurs in precisely the position predicted for it, at the periphery of the remaining verbal morphemes. b. Conditional In Haya the conditional morpheme £813., which may be translated as the English if, states the condition to be met in order for a certain event to take place. B (ibid:1985) suggests that COND should be categorized under MOOD following agreement. Such a suggestion fit into B’s order of morphemes according to Relevance and Generality, with only the change of position between the agreement and COND morphemes in Haya. Whereas agreement is the farthest category in B’s hierarchy and MOOD the last but one, the Haya agreement and conditional morphemes exhibit the reverse order. 0. Negative and Focus B (ibid:176) admits that negation does not quite fit in any of the proposed categories but in some of its "uses negation can resemble (what she is calling) mood, in that it can have the whole proposition in its scope" (words in brackets mine). On the other hand, the category focus is not mentioned by B. This would possibly be categorized under mood also. B defines mood as "how the speaker chooses to put the proposition in the discourse context" (165). As we discussed in section 3.3.8 one of the functions (known thus 244 far) of focus in Haya is emphasis. Such a function of focus fits B’s categorization of rmmxi. If the proposal that negative and focus go under mood go through, then it is appropriate to claim that both NEG and FOC fit B’s prediction. They are located farther away from the stem than most of the non-mood categories in Haya. d. Tense Tense is one of B’s categories. Tense in Haya follow B’s prediction that a tense morpheme will occur approximately in the middle of the other verbal morphemes. e. Infinitive The infinitive is not discussed at all in B’s work. We do not know where B would have placed it. Probably INF would have been categorized as MOOD, but we do not know for sure. 5.3.3.2. The Stem Morphemes a. The Reflexive, Passive and Reciprocal These morphemes fit B’s categorization of voice. B (ibid:20) defines voice as a category "that changes the relation. that the surface subject has to the verb." For example, in the reflexive the subject both performs the action and is affected by the action. These morphemes occurs closer to the stem as predicted by B. 245 b. Causative and Applicative These two morphemes are categorized under valence. According 1x) B such morphemes are highly relevant to "the situation described in the verb stem, since the situation expressed changes according to the number and role of participants in the) situation" (20). Such a description fits the two morphemes in Haya, as described in chapter 3. Moreover these .morphemes occur immediately following the verb-root as discussed in (4.4), which fits the prediction that valence morphemes will occur close to the stem. c. The Reversive and Repetitive Reversive and. Repetitive .morphemes have no apparent classification in B’s list of categories. 5.3.3.3. The Poststem Morphemes (non-stem) a. The indicative/subjunctive morphemes The indicative and subjunctive morphemes are included in B’s mood category. These two morphemes are described as the expression of assertion and non assertion, respectively. Their position in Haya fits B’s prediction in so far as they are further away from the stem. 246 b. Locative, Manner, Question and Intensifier These morphemes have no apparent counterparts in any of B’s categories, although the manner morphemes may possibly be regarded as mood. 5.3.3.4 The Subject Marker and Object Marker In. chapter' 3 section (3.4.1), we proposed, without justification, that the subject markers and object markers in Haya have significantly different syntactic functions. We will discuss this proposal further in this section. Both the SM and the OM encode noun-class features, of the subject and the object in a sentence, respectively. Both of them are, in this sense, agreement markers. However, when we look at their position with respect to the root, it can be seen that the OM is close to the root, at the periphery of the stem, whereas the SM is farther from the root, at the periphery of the. other inflectional morphemes. If both the SM and the OM are agreement markers, then their different positions constitute a counterexample to 8’8 theory. Is there any evidence that the SM and OM in Haya represent different categories, and thus that their different positions might be predicted by B’s theory? In order to answer this question, we will apply ideas developed for the analysis of object markers in Chichewa, a Bantu language related to Haya, by Bresnan & Mchombo (1987; henceforth referred to as B & M1.1l Although B & M develop their analysis within the framework of Lexical-Functional 247 Grammar (Bresnan 1982; this is given in B &.NVs references as Bresnan 1982d), we believe that the arguments they give may stand on their own without extensive discussion of the assumptions underlying this theory. B 8: M distinguish between grammatical and anaphoric agreement. According to 13 & M, morphemes marking grammatical agreement redundantly express agreement features of arguments of the verb. .Morphemes marking anaphoric agreement are incorporated pronominals, and the NPs with which they agree are non-arguments. B & M give two criteria for distinguishing the two, one involving evidence from sentence grammar, the other involving evidence from discourse. We will describe and apply these two to Haya, to argue that the OM in Haya shows syntactic behavior parallel to that of the OM in Chichewa, which constitutes our argument that the OM in Haya is a marker of anaphoric agreement, in B & M’s terms. The first criterion is that of word order. B & M show that in simple transitive sentences, where the verb has no OM, the NP object must immediately follow the verb, whereas the subject may be reordered. Simply stated, when there is no OM, word order is restricted as shown in (744) examples, repeated here as (23): (23) "a. SuVO: Njfichi zi-na -lum -a alenje bees SM-PAST-bite-INDIC hunters ’The bees bit the hunters.’ VOSu: OVsu: VSuO: SuOV: OSuV: 248 Zinaldma alenje njfichi. . . I .A . X Alenje Zinalfima njuchi. . I ' . . . X Zinaluma njachi alenje. .~ . . . z X Njuchi alenje Zinaluma X Alenje njachi zinélfima But when the OM is present, word order is free; that is, all the above orders are possible as shown in their examples, repeated here as (24): (24) "a. b. C. d. SuVO: VOSu: OVsu: VSuO: Njfichi zi-na —wa—lum -a alenje bees SM-PAST-OM-bite-INDIC hunters ’The bees bit them, the hunters.’ Zinawéluma alenje njfichi. . .II .A . Alenje z1nawaluma njuchi. . z I .A . . Zinawaluma njuchi alenje. 249 e. SuOV: Njfichi alenje zinawéluma f. OSuV: Alenje anchi zinawaluma" (7451. The relevant question at this point is: how may these data be accounted for? B & M (745) give five explanations. (a) SM plays two roles: those of grammatical and anaphoric agreement. ‘The pronominal interpretation arises when the subject NP is omitted. As a matter of fact, when the subject NP is omitted in the above examples in (23) and (24) the pronominal interpretation holds (that is, agreement). (b) The OM is used as an incorporated object pronoun and not an object marker. (0) All object NPs in Chichewa appear in post verbal position. Therefore the appearance of a post verbal object in a VP is only possible when there is no OM on the verb. (d) Sentences in Chichewa consist of an optional subject NP, a VP, and an optional topic NP, which are all unordered. Such combinations allows six different orders as schematized below: (25) a. NPsu VP NPt b. NPsu NPt VP 0. VP NPsu NPt 250 d. VP NPt NPsu e. NPt VP NPsu f. NPt VP NPsu where NPsu means noun phrase subject, NPt means noun phrase topic.' (e). Finally, B & M argue that the cooccurrence of OM with an object NP is explained as the 'anaphoric binding of an object pronoun, incorporated in the verb, to a topic NP in S. (746)." The five explanations above help to explain the differences between examples (23) and (24). The SM is both a grammatical agreement marker and an incorporated pronoun, whereas the OM is only an incorporated pronoun. The SM is a grammatical agreement marker when it cooccurs with the NP which controls its 'agreement features; in the absence of this NP it is an incorporated pronoun. The OM is an incorporated pronoun whether or not its controlling NP is present or absent. This controlling NP of the OM is not an object but a topic. The data on reordering constitutes the evidence for this difference between the SM and the OM. If the SM were an incorporated pronoun when cooccuring with its controlling NP, then this NP would be a topic and could be reordered within the VP, even in the absence of an OM. 251 However, it can only be reordered to a position outside of the VP, as indicated in the examples in (23). Therefore, in this case, the SM is purely a grammatical agreement marker. On the other hand, when the OM cooccurs with its controlling NP, this NP can be reordered anywhere, within the VP or outside of it, as in the examples in (24). The OM in this case is an incorporated pronoun. B & M replace the examples (24) with examples (26) below: (26) a. Su[vp v1 TOP: Njfichi zI-né-wélium-a alenje bees SM-PAST—OM-bite-INDIC hunters ’The bees bit them, the hunters.’ b. [vp V] TOP Su: Zinawaluma alenje njfichi. ’they bite them, the hunters, the bees.’ c. TOP[Vp V] Su: Alenje z1nawaluma anchi. ’the hunters, they bite them the bees’ d. [vp V] Su TOP: Zinawaluma njfichi alenje. ’they bite them the bees, the hunters’ 252 e. Su TOP[vp V]: Njfichi alenje zinawaluma ’the bees, the hunters, they bite them’ f. TOP Su[vp V]: Alenje njuchi zinawaluma" (745). ’the hunters, the bees bite them’ B & M’s analysis raises two questions: (747). (1) "Why is gender class agreement required between the topic NP and the incorporated object pronoun?" (2) "Why must the topic NP be anaphorically linked to an INCORPORATED pronoun, which suspiciously' resembles an agreement marker, rather than to an independent pronoun in the object NP position (as in English example I love him dggrlxw_m1_fggh§r1?" (italics omitted). The answer to question 1 above is that person, number, and gender are pronominal categories which universally show agreement in anaphoric relations.u Several examples are given to support this point. We show two of those examples here (10) and (11) repeated here as: (27) (Pointing to a lion lying on the ground) UWU this (class 3) 253 (28) (Pointing to a lion on the ground1X X Ichi this (class 7). In Chichewa the word lion, mkgwgg, belongs to gender class 3. Indicating deixis to a lion, as shown in (27) above, requires the class 3 deictic pronominal form; the class 7 form (28) shown would be used for other class 7 things. These examples indicate that the choice of agreement features of person, number and gender in the anaphoric use of pronominals is independently motivated, and not motivated exclusively by the sentence internal mechanism of agreement. As to the explanation of the use of incorporated pronouns to anaphorically link the topic NP’s to the predicate argument structure, the answer lies in the internal. structure of the language. In Chichewa, "independent object pronouns are used only for introducing new topics or for contrast" (748) (see B & M’s examples (12)-(15) 748-749). As the examples in (29) show, the pattern in Haya is identical to that in Chichewa. Simple transitive sentences in Haya, in which there is no OM on the verb, show fixed word order (the object must immediately follow the verb): (29) a. SuVO: E-njoki zi-ka -lum -a A -ba-hiigi IV-bee IO-RMP-bite-IND IV-Z -hunter ’The bees bit the hunters’ 254 b. VOSu: Zikaluma Enjoki Abahiigi c. OVSu: X Abahiigi zikaluma Enjoki 0" d. VSuO: Zikaluma Enjoki Abahiigi e. SuOV: X Enjoki Abahiigi zikaluma 96 f. OSuV: Abahiigi Enjoki zikaluma But when the OM is present, all the above orders are possible in Haya as well: (30) a. SuVO: E-njoki zi-ka- b; -lum -a A -ba-hiigi IV-bee IO-RMP OM(2) bite-IND IV-2 -hunter ’The bees bit them, the hunters’ b. VOSu: Zikabgluma Enjoki Abahiigi c. OVSu: Abahiigi zikabgluma Enjoki d. VSuO: Zikabgluma Enjoki Abahiigi e. SuOV: Enjoki Abahiigi zikabaluma ‘ f. OSuV: Abahiigi Enjoki zikahaluma 255 In Haya too, as was the case in Chichewa, independent Object pronouns are also used only in introducing new topics or as also is the case in Chichewa, for contrast. (31) a. Omwana akalya ekiliibwa. Olwo yamazile child ate cassava(7). When he finished Ku -ki -ly -a yagya omushule INF-it(7)-eat-IND he went to school ’The child ate the cassava. Having eaten it (the cassava) he went to school.’ ? b. Omwana akalya ekiliibwa. Olwo yamazile child ate cassava(7). When he finished Kulya ebyo yagya omushule eating them(7) he went to school. ’The child ate the cassavas. Having eaten them (something other than cassava) he went to school.’ While example (31.a) is natural in Haya, (31.b) is odd. .The independent pronouns as in Chichewa are interpreted as referring to topics not mentioned in the previous sentence even though they agree with objects of the previous sentence in person, number and gender class. That is, example (31.b) is interpreted as meaning that the child ate several things including cassava. 256 We turn now to the second criterion, that of locality. Locality is defined with respect to the proximity of agreeing elements within the clause structure; local agreement holds between elements of the same simple clause, while non-local agreement may hold between elements of different clauses. In accordance with the assumptions developed in the discussion of word order, locality constitutes another difference between arguments of the verb and topics and focuses linked to the verb 'by :anaphoric agreement. Only non-arguments may be separated from the clauses to which they are linked. B & M illustrate locality in Chichewa with the following examples: I (32) Chigawénga ichi asilikali 5 ganyu terrorist (7) this soldiers of temporary work a -na -Gz -é mtsogoleri wathu kuti SM-REC.PAST-tell-INDIC leader our that I f ' ’ s -a -ngath-e ku -chi -gwir -a not-SM-can -SUBJN INF-OM(71-catch-INDIC ’This terrorist, the mercenaries told our leader they cannot catch him.’ 257 . / I . . . . I .’ A (33) X Chigawenga ichi a31likali a ganyu terrorist (7) this mercenaries ’ I . A .’ a -na -uz -a mtsogoleri wathu kuti SM-REC.PAST-tell-INDIC leader our that I e s -a -ngath-e ku -gw1r -a not-SM-can -SUBJN INF-catch-INDIC ’This terrorist the mercenaries told our leader they cannot catch.’ These examples indicate that an object marker in Chichewa may be used to bind a topic which is outside of the -clause in which the object marker appears, and that such binding is obligatory in Chichewa. Additional evidence that this NP is a topic is seen in the following examples, which indicate that it may be reordered within the topmost clause in the same manner as in the examples above N34.a-c) given earlier to illustrate the reordarability of topics. (341 a. Asilikali é ganyu a -na -fiz -§ mtsogoleri mercenaries SM-REC.PAST-tell-INDIC leader / wathu kuti s -a -ngath-e ku -chi -gwir -a our that not-SM-can -SUBJN INF-OM(7)-catch-INDIC chigawéngé ichi terrorist(7) this ’The mercenaries told our leader that they cannot catch him, this terrorist.’ 258 I b. Asilikali é ganyu chigéwengé ichi mercenaries terrorist(7) this a -na -52 -a mtsogoleri wathu SM-REC.PAST-tell-INDIC leader our 3 -a -ngath-é ku -chi -gwir -a not-SM-can -SUBJN INF-OM(71-catch-INDIC / c.X ? Asilikali 5' ganyu a -na -Gz -é mercenaries SM-REC.PAST-tell-INDIC chigawénga ichi mtsogoleri wathu terrorist(7) this leader our . I .’ .’ kuti s -a -ngath-e ku -chi -gwir -a that not-SM-can -SUBJN INF-OM(7)-catch-INDIC As seen in examples "(34.a) and (34.b1 the topic may be placed at the end of the sentence, or may follow the highest subject. Example (34.c) shows that it may not occur within the VP, following the main verb. This is because only arguments, not non—arguments, may reappear within the VP. With respect to the locality criterion, again the pattern in Haya is identical to that in Chichewa. As in Chichewa, the floating topic NP can be non-local from the OM to which it is linked and the non-local topic shows the same ordering possibilities within its higher clause, as examined in examples (35) and (36), respectively. 259 (35) Omwisi ogu abai shelukali bo okulipwa terrorist (1) this soldiers of temporary work (mercenaries) ba-a -gambil—a omubembezi waitu bati SM-TNs-tell INDIC leader our that ti -ya-shoboil-e ku -mu -kwat -a not-SM—can SUBJN INF-OM(l) catch-INDIC ’This terrorist, the mercenaries told our leader they cannot catch him.’ In (35), the class noun (1) omwisi ’terrorist’ is a floating topic NP. There are three levels of level embedding above class (11 OM which agrees with it. If it is also removed, as was the case in (12), the sentence becomes ungrammatical as shown in (36). (36) X Omwisi ogu abaishelukali bokulipwa terrorist (1) this mercenaries ba-a -gambil-a omubembezi waitu bati SM-TNs-tell INDIC leader our that ti -ya-shoboil-e ku°-mu ~kwat -a not-SM-can SUBJN INF-OM(l) catch-INDIC The ungrammaticality of (36) as was the case in Chichewa, follows from the fact that in Haya too, topicalizations are constructed by anaphoric binding. 260 Concerning the ordering possibilities within the higher clause, in Haya too, the topic NP can appear sentence- finally as in (37.a1, and after the highest subject, as in (37.b) but NOT after the main verb inside the VP, as in (37.0): (371 a. Abaishelukale bo okulipwa ba-a -gambil-a mercenaries SM-TNs-tell -INDIC omubembezi waitu bati ti -ya-shoboil-e leader our that not-SM-can -SUBJN ku -mu . -kwat -a omwisi ogu INF-OM(7) catch-INDIC terrorist (1) this ’This mercenaries told our leader that they cannot catch him, this terrorist.’ b. Abaishelukale bo okulipwa omwisi ogu mercenaries terrorist (11 this ba-a‘ ~gambil-a omubembezi waitu SM-TNs-tell -INDIC leader our ti -ya-shoboil-e ku -mu -kwat —a not—SM-can -SUBJN INF-OM(7) catch-INDIC 261 c.X ? Abaishulukale bo okulipwa ba-a -gambil-a mercenaries SM-TNs-tell -INDIC omwisi ogu omubembezi waitu ° terrorist this leader our bati ti -ya-shoboil-e ku -mu -kwat -a that not-SM-can -SUBJ INF-OM(l) catch-INDIC The application of the Haya data to the criteria established by B 81. M, shows identical results for Chichewa and Haya. The word order criterion shows that both Chichewa and Haya follow a fixed word order: the object immediately following the verb. The locality criterion shows that the floating NP can be non-local from the OM to which it is linked and that non-local topics show the same ordering possibilities within their higher clauses. ‘These results indicates that the (wt in Haya (as well as Chichewa) is not an agreement marker but something else. Let us now turn to the SM. It has been suggested that the OM is always an incorporated pronoun and never a non- referential marker of grammatical agreement (B & M: ibid:755). The relevant question at the moment is what about the SM? It was pointed out earlier that SM is indeed such a marker and it also has a referential use, under appropriate conditions, as an incorporated pronoun. That is, when the SM is used as a grammatical agreement marker, it also agrees with the subject NP, and when the SM is used for anaphoric binding, it functions as a subject topic as 262 shown in B & M’s work as example (321 repeated here as (38). The topic mkango uwu ’this lion’ appears three levels of embedding above its verb- initial sentence position in (38.a), following the highest subject. in (38.b), and in sentence final position in (38.c). (38) a. Mkéngd uwu, alenje a -ku -géniza kutf’ lion(31 this hunters SM-PRES-think that ‘6 -ma -ffina ku -gumdla nyumba yé'mffimu SM(3)-HAB-want INF pull.down house of chief ’This lion, the hunters think that it wants to pull down the chief’s house.’ b. Alenje mkangd uwu a —ka ~ganiza kuti hunters lion(3) this SM-PRES-think that 5 -ma -funa ku —gdmdla nyumba ya mfumu SM(3)-HAB-want INF pull.down house of chief . . .’I I/ c. Alenje a —ka -ganiza kuti u -ma -funa hunters SM-PRES-think that SM(3)-HAB-want ku -gfimula nyumba ya mffimu mkang6 uwu INF pull.down house of chief lion(3) this These examples, show that SM(3) is used as an incorporated pronominal for non-local anaphora to the topic and the other SM is used as a true grammatical agreement marker, a pattern which is also evidenced in Haya. 263 (39) a. Entale egi, abahiigi ni -ba -tekeleza bati lion(91 this, hunters FOC-SM(21—think that NP SUBJ(2) e -yenda ku -teneka enju yo omukama SM(91-want INF pull down house of chief‘ ’This lion, the hunters think that it wants to pull down the chief’s house.’ b. abahiigi entale egi ni -bg -tekeleza bati hunters lion(9) this FOC-SM(21-think that NP SUBJ(21 e -yenda ku -teneka enju yo omukama SM(3)-want INF pull down house of chief c. Abahiigi ni -b§ -tekeleza bati hunters FOC-SM(21—think that NP SUBJ(2) e -yenda ku -teneka enju yo omukama SM(3)-want INF pull down house of chief entale egi lion(9) this As was shown in the Chichewa examples, in Haya too, SM(9) is. used as an incorporated pronominal for non-local anaphora to the topic while the other SM (SM(2) underlined) is used as a 264 true grammatical agreement. marker agreeing in (class and number with the NP subject. The SM is also used referentially when it does not co- occur with a nominal to which it has to agree in class and number as exemplified in both Chichewa (40) and Haya (41). (40) Chichewa (41) a. gi-na -lum -a alenje SM-PAST-bite-INDIC hunters b. gi-na -wa-lum -a SM-PAST-OM-bite-INDIC c. gi-na -wa-lum -a ' alenje SM-TNs-OM-bite-INDIC hunters Haya a. Zi-ka -lum -a abahiigi SM-TNs-bite-IND hunters b. zi-ka -ba-lum -a SM-TNs-OM-bite-IND c. Zi-ka -ba-1um -a abahiigi SM-TNs-OM-bite-IND hunters 265 In such cases the SM is itself the subject in the absence of the overt NP subject. As nicely summarized by Moshi (1985:139) the ”subject has neither something to control nor something by which it is controlled." This adds evidence to B & M’s treatment of SM as both used for grammatical agreement and anaphoric binding. Bresnan and Mchombo present evidence that in Chichewa the SM is both a grammatical agreement marker and an incorporated pronoun, whereas the OM is exclusively an incorporated pronoun. This evidence also exists in Haya. (Similar conclusions have been reached by Byarushengo, et al., (1976) in their study of tone in Haya; cf. B & M (1987: 751, fn.13).) The difference between the two suggests an account of their different positions with respect to the root within B’s theory. The SM functions partially as an agreement role in determining its position. ij the SM is regarded primarily as an agreement marker, then it may be seen that it occupies the position predicted by B’s theory. What about the OM? The evidence suggests that it is exclusively an incorporated pronoun, a category which is not included in B’s list of inflectional categories whose linear order is predicted by Relevance and Generality. However, a pronoun represents an argument of the verb and this observation may indicate a connection between a pronominal inflectional category and valence, even though valence, in B’s account, refers to morphemes which alter argument structure, not just represent it. (29) Insofar as this 266 connection may be made, the position of the OM with respect to the root fits B’s prediction concerning the position of a valence morpheme. This explanation is problematic, since the SM may also function as an incorporated pronoun under Bresnan & Mchombo’s analysis, but B’s theory may be considered to offer a tentative account of the connection (if any) between the different functions of the SM and the OM and their different positions in the verb. Thus far, a distinction has been made between the OM and SM. It has been established that the SM functions as a grammatical marker as well as an anaphoric binder. Such an analysis based on the Haya data supports B’s prediction and B 8: M’s analysis. We have examined the SM and OM in Haya and concluded that they show different syntactic behavior. What about the reflexive marker, RFM? As seen in chapter 3, the RFM resembles the OM in that it immediately precedes the verb root and refers to an object of the verb. It differs in that it does not encode the noun-class of the object it refers to. The question now is: does the RFM behave like an incorporated pronominal (in B & M’s terms), like the OM? The RFM, unlike the OM, cannot occur with the NP that it refers to: (42) X A -ba-hiigi ba-ka -e -tem-a a -ba-hiigi IV 2 hunter 2 RMP-RFM-cut-IND IV 2 hunter ’hunters cut themselves hunters’ 267 Therefore, neither the criterion of word order nor the criterion of locality used earlier to argue that an OM which cooccurs with the object it refers to is an incorporated pronominal can be used with the RFM, since both of these criteria. rely crucially’ on the» OM cooccurring with the object it refers to. In the next section we raise an issue which we have not discussed so far, concerning our earlier analysis of stems and non-stem morphemes? How may we further justify this analysis? HOW’ about the traditional approach of derivational and inflectional morphemes. Does this approach concur with B’s approach. These are some of the questions we try to answer next. 5.3.3.5. Split between sets of morphemes In chapter 4 we split the Haya verb morphemes into two groups: the stem mOrphemes and non-stem morphemes, both occurring before and after the verb root. The non-stem morphemes. may" be further divided. into prestem morphemes (43.a) and poststem morphemes (43.b): (43).non-stem morphemes IV a. COND - SM - NEG2 - TNS - (NEG3 - (INF (NEGZ)) - X- NEGl FOC b. x - IND - LOC - MNR - Q - INT SUBJ 268 These two schemata may be shown according to their relation to the Root, (marked x in 43) as shown in (44). Since the Root is obligatory, we will show its cooccurrence with the prestem and proststem morphemes. W (44) COND - SH - NEGZ - TNS - NEG} - (INF (NE62)) - ROOT -{IND }- LDC - HNR - 0 ‘ INT NEGI SUBJ FM The stem morphemes are arranged as follows: (451 RFM - ROOT -{CAUS}-{PASS}- APL - RPT (DOl)(1031 APL RCP Each set of morphemes really constitutes a separate set as exemplified in (46): (461 a. a -ba-laa-shal-a -ha IV-Z -IF -cut-IND-LOC ’who will cut here’ b. ku -gul-a -ki-kwo INF-buy-IND-Q -INT ’to indeed buy what’ 269 0. ti -mu—chumb-ile NEGl-Zp-cook -RP ’you haven’t cooked’ Examples (46.a-c) indicate that non-stem morphemes may cooccur on their own without stem morphemes. This supports our discussion in chapter 2 of the non-stem morphemes in terms of their relationship to the stem, without reference to the internal structure of the stem. In addition, the stem morphemes were described without reference to non-stem morphemes. In chapter 4, the claim was made that, with very few exceptions, the cooccurrence restrictions on morphemes hold between morphemes belonging exclusively to one group or the other, the non-stem morphemes or the stem morphemes. We again show some examples to clarify this point. The begin with examples which show the cooccurrence restrictions of the non-stem morphemes: (47) a. X ka -ti -ba-li-bon-a COND-NEGl-Z -NF-see-IND b. X ba-ta -li-ku -lum -a 2 -NEG2-NF-INF-bite-IND Examples (47.a-b) are ill-formed because in (47.a) COND and NEGl are mutually exclusive and therefore cannot cooccur. 270 In (47.b) it is shown that INF cannot cooccur with the SM, NF or NEGZ. The examples above are repeated here with necessary modified requirements and they are well—formed. (48) 8. ka -ba-li-bon-a COND-2 -NF-see-IND ’when they see’ b. ti -ba-li-bon-a NEG1-2 -NF-see-IND ’they wont see’ (49) a. ba-ta -li-lum -a 2 -NEG2-NF-bite—IND ’they should not bit (in the near future)’ b. ku -lum -a INF-bite-IND ’biting/to bite’ Furthermore, the stem morphemes by themselves may have noun formation morphology attached rather than the verbal morphology discussed in chapter 3. Noun formation morphology is exemplified as follows: (50) (51) (52) Note phonological changes as described in chapter 2. a. 8.. a. that examples (50.b) and 271 ku -kol -a INF-work-IND ’to work’ 0 -mu-koz -i IV-l -work-DA ’worker’ ku -eelez-a INF-serve—IND ’to serve’ a-ba-eelez-i N-2 -serve-DA ’servants/helpers” ku -lwal -a INF-be ill-IND ’to be ill’ o-mu-lwail-e IV-I-sick-PAT ’sick person (patient)’ (52.b) are subject to Note also that the vowels following the verb stem in these examples 272 imply broadly the doer of the action (DA) in example (50.b), (51.b), and patient or sufferer of action (PAT) in example (52.b). The following examples show various stem morphemes which may appear with this noun-formation morphology: (53) a. o -mu-e -yeg -es -elez-o —-> (omwegeshelezo) IV-3 -RFM-learn-CAUS-RPT -RST ’teachings (i.e. religious teachings)’ b. o -mu-sig -il -w -a IV-3 -trust-APL-PASS-IND ’steward/trustee’ c. a -ba-shab-iliz-i IV-2 -beg -RPT -DA ’beggars’ The above discussion of the order of the Haya verb morphemes fits B’s prediction of the "mirror-image’ order. In Haya where the ROOT is lbcated in the middle of the Haya verb morphemes, we suggest that the most relevant morphemes occur closest to the root and the least relevant occur at the greatest distance from the root. Therefore the stem morphemes are more relevant to the root than the prestem or poststem morphemes (cf. the schemata of the verbal morphemes of Haya in chapter 3 (53). These two groups of morphemes are reminiscent of the more familiar division made between 273 inflectional and derivational morphology.“ We may tentatively propose that the non-stem morphemes are inflectional morphemes, whereas the stem morphemes are derivational. As we pointed out in.(5.2.1.1), B’s idea is that the inflectional/derivational distinction is a continuum rather than a sharp division. Note that support for this idea comes from the cooccurrence restrictions which hold between the groups. They appear to be restrictions which hold between morphemes which are at the border of the continuum between inflectional and derivational. This is where you expect to find morphemes which behave somewhat like inflectional. morphemes and somewhat. like derivational morphemes. We will here discuss one such morpheme, the subjunctive. The SUBJ morpheme must cooccur with SM or OM. (cf. examples (56.a,b)). (Note that when it cooccurs with OM, there is always an option of SM). This, therefore, is an instance of a cooccurrence restriction holding between a non-stem morpheme (SUBJ) and a stem morpheme (OM). Note that the SUBJ morpheme occurs immediately after the stem morphemes of the root. (54) a. mu -ba -twal-e o -mu-shule SM(2p)-OM(2)-take-SUBJ IV-9 -school ’you (pl) should take them to school’ 274 b. ba -twal-e omushule OM(2)-take-SUBJ school ’take them to school’ 5.4. Carstairs theory We would like to conclude this chapter by briefly discussing Carstairs’ (1983) theory of the Paradigm Economy Principle (PEP) which poses an interesting observation that is relevant to this study. Carstairs investigated the distribution of the inflexional resources available in some word-classes or part of speech among the languages of world class (16); and found out that the ’inflexional resources’ as he calls them, do not approach the upper limit, but rather the minimum. In other words, languages which have a large number' of inflectional morphemes, and therefore a large number of potential combinations of these morphemes, do not exhibit the maximum number of combinations but rather the minimum. These inflexional resources includes for example nominal and verbal morphemes, among others. With evidence from five languages-Latin, Hungarian, Zulu (a Bantu language), Dyirbal and. German, and by applying what he called the "traditional notion of paradigm" he found out that those languages do not approach the "logical maximum" but are at or close to the minimum possible inflexional resources. As an example, suffix combinations of classical Latin were worked out, totalling 1,371,686,400 possibilities but only 5 actually occur (119). With these results, 275 Carstairs hypothesizes that "there exists a real tendency towards ’paradigm economy’ - towards keeping the total paradigms for any word class close to the logical minimum" (127). The question is, is this prediction borne out in Haya? With 23 morphemes in the Haya verb (see chapter 3 (7)), it is logically possible to have a maximum of 1,334,832 morpheme combinations, reached by multiplying the 22 morphemes with the two or more possibilities of appearance or non-appearance of each category, for those which are optional. These logical possibilities extend from two obligatory morphemes within a single stem - . . . (55) STEM + IND} SUB - to the maximum. The maximum possible number of morphemes in a verb stem is approximately 12,960. (see rule 71 chapter 4). This number is approximately 9.7% of the total logical maximum combination. Haya therefore confirms Carstairs theory, as does B’s work (see chapter 3 of her book), that there exists a real tendency of keeping the total paradigms for any word class to the minimum. 5.6. Summary and conclusion Testing B’s hypothesis with Haya data tends to confirm her work, with minor modifications. Although it is not very clear whether B groups the'voice and valence categories with 276 derivational morphemes, we have done so on the basis of evidence internal to Haya. Given this conclusion, this study supports B’s prediction concerning hierarchy and order and more generally RELEVANCE and GENERALITY in so far as the hierarchy and order. predictions follow from the two principles. We have also discussed the observation that the OM in Haya fits the prediction in B’s model only when it is regarded as an incorporated object pronoun, considered as a valence marker, following the evidence derived from B & M’s (1987) study. These conclusions support our earlier general conclusions reached in chapter 4, that semantic, syntactic and pragmatic factors determine the linear- order of morphemes. Syntactic factors have not only been discussed in chapter 4 but also in this chapter when discussing among, other things, B & M’s proposals of the analysis and order of the OM and SM. Finally, we mentioned in chapter 4, that the linear order of morphemes can be barred by'the meaning derived through combinations of morphemes well formed syntactically but not accepted culturally (pragmatic factors). 277 CHAPTER 5 ENDNOTES 1 B’s theory has been reviewed by Aronoff (19871 and Walker (1986), but has not (as far we know) been applied to any Bantu language. 3 These ways are not discrete, but fall along a continuum, whose intermediate points are not discussed here. Cf. B: Chap.2.1, in particular 12.) 3 This example illustrates the problematic character of Generality, since a particular category can be made more or less general depending on how the "appropriate semantic and syntactic category" is defined. She the Latin motion verb example, if the "appropriate .. . . category" is defined as all verbs, as she defines it, then motion prefixes are not general; however, if the category is defined as all motion verbs, then it is. It seems to us that Generality is most useful as a relative notion: one morpheme is more or less general than another with respect to a particular syntactic category.) ‘ B mentions three other independent predictions: language acquisition, language change tuul word order. As for language acquisition, B notes that children acquire categories that are closer to the stem before those further away from the stem. For example, according to the child language literature, aspectual distinctions on 'verbs are made long before person/number and agreement distinctions. (Antinucci and Miller 1976, Stephany 1981, Simoes and Stoel Gammon 1979, Bloom et al 1980). 278 Evidence cited for language change indicates that changes occur in the exterior positions vs the interior positions with respect to the stem (Bybee and Brewer 1980). Finally, a comment is also made that the order of morphemes within a word reflect an earlier ordering of words within a sentence (Givon 1971, Vennemann 1973). We did not get into details of these predictions for Haya. 5 See Aronoff’s (1987:117) reservations on these principles. 6 We will return to this point during the discussion of the application of B’s theory to Haya data (Section 5.3.3). 7 The thirteen least Relevant categories are IV, COND, NEG, FOC, SM, TNS, INF, IND, SUBJ, LOC, MNR, Q, and INT. The ten highly Relevant categories are: RFM, DO, IO, CAUS, BEN, PASS, RCP, ST, RPT and RV. 8 A fourth type not discussed by B is what Comrie (1981:39) has called polysynthetic (or incorporating). In a polysynthetic language it is possible to combine a large number of morphemes, lexical or grammatical, into a single word, often corresponding to a whole sentence of English e.g. Eskimo etc. (Ibid:42) (see details). 9 Garo is one of Perkins sample languages (see B ibid:215). It is a Sine—Tibetan type of languages (Burlington 1961). 1° However, note that B acknowledges that an exhaustive survey of derivational morphology was not undertaken . 279 that "there are many more possible derivational categories than the ones mentioned . . ." (29). 11 Chichewa is a Bantu language spoken in East Central Africa - particularly in Malawi and its neighboring countries of Mozambique, Zambia, and Zimbabwe, where it is also known as Chinyanja. See B & M (1987:7431 fn3. 13 B & M cite one other piece of evidence for the pronominal use of object markers, that they may be used as resumptive pronouns in the repair of island violations (752- 764); we will not discuss this use of the object marker here. 13 Givon (1976) proposed that, "in the historical development of Bantu, subject and object pronouns used for reference to topics (’The man, he came’) became cliticized, and then morphologically bound to the verbs (’The man he- came’); B & M (19871741). 1‘ There is a current debate going on as to whether derivation and inflection must be separated or not. Scalise (1986:1021 lists all those who regard inflection in grammar as operating in the syntactic component (the so-called ’Weak Lexalist Hypothesis’ (WLH)) as morphologists who assume that Derivation and inflection are different in nature. These morphologists include Chomsky and Halle ((19681, Chomsky (1970), Siegel (1974) Aronoff (1976), Allen (1978.a), AnderSOn (1982) and others. A list of those who maintain that_ they. are not different and therefore must. not be 280 separated is long. the reader is referred to Scalise (ibid:102). Scalise (ibid:103-115) lists 8 reasons in favor of the division of the two blocks of rules. CHAPTER 6 CONCLUSION 6.1. Summary In this chapter we summarize the major findings of this study and state some suggestions for future research. We also state the contribution this study has made in the field of linguistics. In chapter 1, we provided a brief overview of the Haya language, including its .classification and dialects. We stated the major goals of this study, which are: (1) to identify the Haya verb morphemes and their linear order by answering the question: is the linear ordering of the Haya verb morphemes predictable? (2) to discover the. factors which determine the ordering of the Haya verb morphemes, and (3) to determine whether the factors in (2) above, follow B’s theory. In the last section of chapter 1 we discussed the sources of the data and the organization of this dissertation. In chapter 2 we provided a phonological sketch of Haya, which includes the phonological inventory, a word on tones, the distribution of consonant and vowels, the phonological 281 282 rules and finally a word on syllable structure. Only those phonological aspects which are relevant to the Haya verb morphology are discussed. . In chapter 3, we identified 23 Haya verb morphemes and we discussed the functions of these morphemes. In chapter 4 we discussed four factors which determine the cooccurrence restrictions of the verb morphemes. These factors are (1) phonological, (2) syntactic, (31 semantic, and (4) pragmatic. For example, certain morphemes cannot cooccur because they are phonologically identical also, some morphemes have cooccurrence restrictions because they clash semantically. Such extensions includes the passive (-w-_) and stative (-ik-). In identifying the four factors above, we have taken a broader perspective on the interaction of the various language components. Our work as we have mentioned earlier supports other linguists (for example, Aronoff 1981, Anderson 1982, 1986, Scalise 1986, Selkirk 1982) in arguing that morphology interacts with other parts of grammar, namely, phonology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics. In chapter 5 we tested B’s theory with Haya data. We found' out that Haya data tends to support B’s work, with minor modificationsQ For example, by grouping voice and valence categories with the stemt morphemes in Haya B’s prediction concerning hierarchy and order (of her 50 language sample) and. more generally, RELEVANCE and GENERALITY, is supported. 283 6.2. Suggestions for further research. As we mentioned in chapter 1 and chapter 5, our work is a single language approach, but this study may be—expanded and modified to include ten or more Bantu languages. Such expansion of the data base would ensure that all the morphemes that are most common in Bantu languages are covered and the linear order and or co-occurrence restrictions is determined. Such a large data base could then be used to test B’s theory such as her theory about the frequency of occurrence of inflectional categories as verbal morphemes. Such a prediction, as we pointed out could not be evaluated in a study of one language. In addition to that, a huge data base gathered from ten or more Bantu languages could be compared to another ten or more non—Bantu languages and the results could be related to B’s theory. 6.3. Contribution Despite the limited nature of this work, it has made several contributions. 1. This study as far as we know is the first to pub B’s theory to the test. In so doing we have fulfilled B’s invitation which she made to readers, to test her predictions: ”no systematic test of these predictions has been carried out, but the reader is invited to apply his or 284 her knowledge of the languages of the world to test these predictions" (page 20). 2. Although this study used B’s (ibid) general principles of: RELEVANCE and GENERALITY, it differs in the method applied. Whereas B examined the inflectional morphology of verbs in 50 languages, our methodology was to investigate one Bantu language (Haya) in detail, a language which was not represented in her sample (a modified version of Parkins (1980)). It was shown that Haya has all of B’s categories along with additional categories that did not fit into B’s categorization. Moreover, because Haya is a highly agglutinating language, it was easier to see the order of morphemes relative to the stem. This study has therefore added to 3’3 data. 3. In addition, because this work is embodied in a recent theory of the linear order of morphemes, it will (hopefully) be of interest to morphologists in general and African morphologists in particular. That is, this work has added on to the growing literature on morphology, particularly on African languages. APPENDICES . APPENDIX A A FEW EXAMPLES OF PRESTEM MORPHEMES IN USE A FEW EXAMPLES OF PRESTEM MORPHEMES IN USE EE.L.A.T.I.Y.E§. Eibala kalikwenda lwa Bunyana Her name is Beloved-one Enkuku eyonakateka omumutwe! What knuckles which I tapped her head with! Akati k’engoma ilikwekonya Litle scourge of fearful rulers Aina enkoni y’Omukoma-mpango His quarter-staff wraps up the matter Manti leba nibo balikumweteza Certainly they bring it on themselves Mwenda-ngabo atumilwemu Nkwele A shield-breaker sent by King Nkwele 285 286 Omwibobo elya wakakya At the iron bars that chime a warning Ekyalabile mbele ekya Kalemela First to bear fruit of Kalemera Eyayoky’ engoma yomukama ali mulenzi He who burned a royal drum while still a youth Olunyana lwawe olwo wagisize Your favorite calf which you gave the special pasture to Waitu olwo wagisize Kijongo My lord the one you pastured specially nat Kijongo Ndiwa Bushalita na Muchwampaka#Ekyakukwata I am of bushalita and the Decider-of-cases#The -matter-that- holds-you-in-its-grasp Alikubandama kegy’ eigulu One who knealt to give his arrow flight CONDITIONAL Kabatamu zeleba zaikiliza When they consulted there the oracle saw and favorably spoke 287 Nkahulila kayagobile nyalulembo nyalulembo nyalulembo I heard the king as he reached his palace forecourt forecourt forecourt Kayagobile omwilembo ly’Abakulwa bonene Kayanga Kayanga When he entered the Bakulwa forecourt at Kayanga (?) Kayachwile kuleba ati Omumuti omumushasha Suddenly he glanced up in a mushasha tree Kabashabukile Kyenyabasa When they had crossed the ford at Kyenyabasa Kayaleba ati Kamachumu yaija yabona omuli Lwanda n’omuli Busingo He looked thus at Kamachumu he came and saw into Lwanda and Busingo Mh Kayatukula yayema ahali Lutenge yakokoloma When it ran red he stood up at Lutenge and crowed like a rooster Emihambi kelikulaba omubishansha When the arrows began to fly among the leaves of the banana plants ‘ Kayahikile omukashekela mwonene When he arived at laugh-at village 288 Kabalengile bwankya When they judged it to be daybreak Komubembe Tibagiteile Since they did not beat the Leper Kabagaba omumugamba When they piled them in the back pantry Kabaganywa gabakwata When they drank the beer it took hold of them Kayayetengya bati Atem’ ente niwe When he’d draw attention to himself with kingly gestures they’d say The slasher of cattle herds is you Mwakutengya olubingo tulutema bwoli bwoli Draw our attention to a bamboo grove and we’ll slash it truly truly Kayayetengya bati Atem’ente niwe When he’d draw attention to himself with kingly gestures they’d say The slasher of cattle gerds is you NEGAIIV S Nka naba ntakujumile Never have I said an angry word to you 289 Mbali obwile bwakyela Nor since the dawn last lightened have we fought Ati Omuloto naba ntamanyile I did not see a dream she said Waitu tinabaliz’ ekindi My lord I did not ask another word Tiyabaliz’ ekindi He did not ask other word on that account Kolaba otamanya okwo agolala If you really don’t know how he sets things straight Tiyabaliz’ ekindi He did not need to ask another word Endulu tiyalek’ endulu The cry did not leave off the cry muses Present Universal Habitual. Ninye omu Kishenye ekya minazi I was there at Sandy-spot of the munazi trees 290 Ati Obuso inye nitongana ekilamyo I beg to tender my respect in his presence Omushaija ntukuganyila Fellow we pity you (?1 Nagolol’ata omukama? How does the king set things straight? Abantu natema nemiholo angili nasiga omubazi? Does he out people up with machetes or does he administer medicines? Angili leba nanyuka omubili Or does he massage the body? Lwamanumi nagoloza ebintu The owner-of-bulls straightens things with wealth Twasiga nayesha obulaza We left him playing stockpile strategy in makala Omukama nayesh’ ekigumba The king plays the strategy of cattle herd Emboni nazihanga Lutwiga But his pupils point to the slavegirl long-necked Lutwiga 291 Ati Waitu nintongana My lord I beg a favor Waitu obuso nintongana ekilamyo My lord I beg to tender my respect in his presence Obuso ninyija kuitayo omwiyo I’m coming soon to order that the court be cleared Ninkulamya ekyobutini I tender my respect in words of fearfulness Ninshuba ninkutaho ekyobwegula moyo As well I set before you words to buy my life Immediate Past Nakutawe ekitebe oli w’enketo I place before you a throne to sit Kisa-nshone nayesig’ eiganzi She-who-can-make-a-man-feel-shame is certain that she’s loved 292 Akakazi kangutuz’ omuloto gwange Well this little one stopped my dream for me Kati Baba wantulaki? She says Why chastize me love? Kuba wangutuz’ omuloto gwange Because you broke my dream for me Buti Walotaki mbandwa? What was your dream spirit-medium? Waitu walotaki embandwa? What was your dream our lord spirit medium? Omunaga bwateka ahakyoto The placed an earthenware pot among the flames Akashumu omungemu bwandagaza With the pith of-a banana plant they wipe my hands Eichumu bwateka ahakilindi My spear they placed on the sharpening block Ekilele bwatamu amabale My calabash they cleaned inside with stones 293 Otwalw’ otwo kulangaiza Banana beer to sip relax and thind with Amajuta bwanteka ahamulundo With butterfat they rubbed my shins Near Past Obukazi tibwakeile kilekyo The ladies did not delay that day Tibabaliz’ ekindi They did not ask another word Nilwo nasokile omuly’entale And then I descended into the wilderness of lions Omu Kamezile-nfuka At Hoe—swallower field Ah Muhimbile-ngundu Ah Protector Bull Bati Omushaija anyoile amalwa obukyakya The man’s drunk beer at dawn they said 294 Omushaija anyoile amalwa olukyakya The man’s drunk beer at dawn Nkome kaisho kaigaile The One-eyed Binding -rope Ati Wenene asingile njuki? He said In which house is he now? Asingile njuki? In which house is he now? Ayegekeile Njwanga He’s leaned against his wife Njwanga Lwa Mugimbile-ngundu Oh Protector Bull Tuhulil’ endulu Kalabagaine We heard a cry from Kalabagaine village Bahi Ikilakulya kihigile bwankya They said the What-will-chew-you-up troop went to hunt at dawn Olwo yakasingile akyaija When he had come 295 Olwo yakamutaileho amaisho When he set eyes on him Remote Past Nkabanduka omwibanga ly’empumi I climbed the mountain of hyenas Nkataho omutana gw’etaba And then a roll of tobacco Nkahulila ati Telikunda ya Byabato I heard him call Never-gives-in child of children’s-matters Nkakuba omuti I climbed a tree Nkagya Buganda I went to Uganda Nkatabala Buzinja Nagya natabala Bunyoro I raided in Buzinza and then raided in Bunyolo Nkaitamu omuntu I killed a man there Akabikila Makuba Ngimbwa He summoned Causer-of—retreats Obliged-one Nkahulila Omuhamba I heard a Muhamba Nkabona ati Yoi yoi yoi yoi I heard someone say Yo yo yo yo He’s speared you Omuli Ikunyu mwonene nkabona In Ikunyu village I saw Obulwano bweila bukankwasa enkoju ahalisho A longstanding feud has fixed a scar upon my eye Nkahulila wenene Omunyihangilo I heard the Munyaihangilo Akataishula abashaija tukalwana And free his men to fight Infinitive/ "Immaginary" Marker Kusisimuka kitwe yampinda Awake I started with a big head roaring 297 Waitu bwatamu otwo kulangaiza My lord they poured in just enough to sip relax and think with Nikyo kwija kundagila omutambi Which is why I came to be directed to a healer Nikwo kwija Which is why I came Kutola mugambwage yange To catch my Well-spoken-of-thing Ahi tata kugya kushwela mawe He said When my father went to court my mother Okugyeta mashayaye Calling it a broken-mouthed vessel Nakuba nimpulila Nyamunyalila Ishanga I would be hearing the Urinataor-in-the-marsh Nakuba nimpulila Nyamwitwa ababi I would have heard Ruined-by—the-ugly Ati Bakwitayo enjobe They must have killed a bushbuck (?) there he said 298 Bakwitayo eikombo They must have killed a mongoose there Nakuba nimpulila lwa magabe I would be hearing the Apportioner-of-trails Nakuba nimpulila bilimu lw’ enanga I would be hearing the Sound—of—an-enanga Bakwitayo omunyima They must have killed an ’omunyima’ there Nakuba nimpulila ebijumilo nkenzaile I would be hearing groaning like a calving cow APPENDIX B SOME STEM MORPHEMES: RESTRICTION OF OCCURRENCE BASED ON VERB TYPES 2 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Some STEM jum- shagal— yongel- igan- kul- ikiliz- laga- gendelel- bon- lek- hindul— hulul- nigaal— bik- olool- yetab- hakan— hik- baz- shab- stem morphemes: based on verb types. restriction of occurrence RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT ’abuse’ X + ’accompany’ X + ’add to’ X XX ’be equal’ X + ’grow’ ~' X + ’agree’ X X ’promise’ X + ’progress’ X + ’see’ + + ’leave’ X + ’alter’ X + ’prune (coffee1’. X + ’get anger’ ' X + ’announce death’ + + ’answer’ X + ’answer (call)’ X + ’argue + + ’arrive’ X + ’ask’ 3 X + ’ask for’ X + 299 ¥ X + + + + + + 21 22 23 24 25 27 28 29 3O 31 32 33 34 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 STEM sisiimuk- 8&8- shob- gamb- jumbik- shaabul- boigol- gumb- yog— b- zaal- banz- jugut- hend- shombf inam- log- hangu— lum- shaalil- ilagul— hum- huut— fuu— yatan- 300 ’wake up’ ’go bad’ ’go bad ’say’ ’bake in ashes’ ’bail out of water’ ’bark’ ’barren’ ’bathe’ :ben ’bear children’ ’begin’ ’blow’ ’break’ ’carry things’ ’bend’ ’witch’ ’big, become’ ’bite’ ’become bitter’ ’become black’ ’become blind’ ’blow’ ’blow noise’ ’border on RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 STEM tiil— yat- gutul- iky- leet- lel- monyok- ey- yombek- yoky- yak- hy- balul- tond- baluk- baluul- ziik- tem- gul- yet- zing- heek- shutul- tweek- twaal- ’borrow’ ’break’ ’break’ 301 ’breathe’ ’bring’ ’bring up (child1’ ’break off’ ’sweep’ ’build’ ’burn’ ’take away’ ’become cooked’ ’burst open’ ’boast of’ ’burst’ ’retell’ ’bury’ ’butch, 'buy’ ’call’ ’fold’ cut down’ ’carry on back (child)’ ’carry in arms (child)’ ’carry on head (child)’ ’take’ RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT 71 72 73 74 75 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 9o 91 92 93 94 95 STEM shumul- lekel- ky- bing- kanj- nokel- lond- jwal- juul- chuchul- ij- lug- chumb- kolol— bal- shweek- yatik- yajul- shaabuk- bandam- lil- lime lumik- tamb- log- 302 ’castrate’ ’cease’ ’Xcease of rain’ ’chase’ ’chew’ ’choke’ ’choose’ ’clothe’ ’take off (clothes1’ ’comb hair’ ’come’ ’come from’ ’cook’ ’caugh’ ’count’ ’cover’ ’crack’ ’craw (of child)’ ’cross over’ ’crouch’ icrye ’cultivate’ ’cupp’ ’cure’ ’curse’ RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT I X + + + + + t 4. + + + XX XX XX XX + 4. XX 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 STEM shal- zin- ilagul- 8W- f- beiy- sing- nYY' yetong— kelelelw- imuky- shuuk- shush- end- songok- lem- iy- bul- ihul- fundul- tamw- gab- kol- hanantuk- t... 303 ’cut up’ ’sing’ ’get black/dark’ ’fall’ ’die’ ’deceive’ ’defeat’ ’defecate’ ’deny’ ’delay’ ’depart’ ’descend’ ’look like’ ’desire’ ’descend’ ’be difficult for’ ’dig (farms) uproot’ ’get lost, disappear’ ’dish up food’ ’open’ ’dislike’ ’give, sacrifice, divide’ ’do’ ’get down’ ’go home/draw wather’ RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT * X + + + + X X + XX 4. XX XX XX + + 304 STEM ' RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT 121 loot— ’dream’ + + + X X + + 122 nyw- ’drink’ X + + + X X X 123 toony— ’drip’ X + + + X X + 124 tamiil- ’get drunk’ + + + X X X + 125 ikal- ’dwell’ X + + + + X + 126 ly- ’eat’ X + + + + XX X 127 laal- ’go without food’ X + XX XX X X XX 128 gool- ’embarass’ ' X + + X X + X 129 hing- ’exchange’ X + + X + X X 130 shoboolol- ’explain’ X + + + + + + 131 laaz- ’extinguish’ X + + + + X + 132 lemw- ’get fired/fail’ + + X X X + X 133 kom- ’wrat, fasten’ + + + + + + 2+ 134 gomok- ’become fat’ X + + X X X + 135 mal- ’finish’ X + + X + + + 136 honder- ’follow’ X + + X + + + 137 kuntil— ’foot fall’ X + + + X X + 138 lagul- ’foretell’ + + + + + + + 139 tang- ’forbid’ X + + + + + + 140 nywan-i ’become friends X + + + + + + 141 yeb- ’forget’ X + + + + X + 142 tiin- ’frighten’ X + + X + + + 143 kalang- ’fry’ X + + + + + + 144 ijul- ’become full’ X + + X X X + 145 igut- ’become full’(with X + + + + X + food) STEM RV CAUS BEN ST PASS RCP RPT 146 sheeny- ’gather firewood’ X + + + + X + 147 h- ’give’ ‘X + X X X + X 148 lagil- ’give a message’ X + + + + + + 149 gend- ’go’ X + + + + X + 150 ebembel- ’go ahead’ X ‘+ + X + + + Ker: X ill-formed XX ill-formed but becomes a different lexical item + well formed (blank space) B IBLI OGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY ALLEN, M. 1978a. Morphological investigations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University (MS Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut. 1978b. The morphology of negative prefixes in English. North Eastern Linguistic Society, Toronto, Canada. 8:1-11. 1980. Semantic and phonological consequences of boundaries: A morphological analysis of compounds, M. Arnoff and M. L. Kean (eds.) Juncture, Anna Libri, 9- 27. ANDERSON , HENNING. 1980 . Morphological change: toward typology. Historical Morphology, ed. by J. Fisiak, The Hague: Mounton. ANDERSON, STEPHEN. R. 1982. Where’s Morphology?, Linguistic inquiry 13, 571-612. 1986. Morphological theory. To appear in Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey. ANTINUCCI, FRANCESCO and RUTH MILLER. 1976. How children learn to talk about what happened. Journal of Child- Language 3. 169-189. ARONOFF, MARK. 1976. Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. 1980. The relevance of productivity in a synchronic description of word formation. Historical Morphology, edited by J. Fisiak, The Hague: 1980. and ANSHEN, FRANK. 1981. Morphological productivity and phonological transparency. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 26.63-72. 1982. Potential words, Actual words, Productivity, and Frequency. Preprints of the plenary session papers. The XIIIH1 International Congress of Linguistics, Tokyo, 141-148. 306 307 1987. Review: Morphology: A study of relationship between meaning and form, by J. L. Bybee. 1985. Language. 63.1:115-129. ARNOTT, D. W. 1970 The nominal and verbal system of Fula. Oxford Clarendon Press. ASHTON, EDITH O. - 1944. Swahili Grammar (including intonation). Longman Group Ltd. et al. 1954. A Luganda Grammar. London: Longmans, Green. AYERS, M. DONALD. 1986. English words from Latin and Greek elements. The University of Arizona Press. TUCSON. BAMMERSBERG, A. 1984. A sketch of diachronic English morphology. Regensburg: Press. BARBER, E. J. W. 1975. Voice--beyond the passive. Bulletin of Linguistic Science 1.16-24. BARLOW, RUFFEL A. 1951. Studies in Kikuyu grammar and idiom, Edingurgh. Blackwood. BATIBO, HERMAN M. 1980. Review: Nurse, Description of Sample Bantu Languages of Tanzania In Journal of Africa Languages and LlngUISthS. 2.2:172-176. BAUER, LAURIE. 1979. Against word based morphology. Linguistic Inquiry 10.3: 508-9. 1983. English word formation. London. Cambridge University Press. BEARD, ROBERT E. 1977 On the extent and nature of irregularity in the lexicon. Lingua 42:305-341. BEAUCHAT, P. D. 1963/64. A restatement of the Zulu Verb Conjugation. African Studies. 22.4:137-169, 23.1:36-49 ptz, 2302:67-87 pt3. 1966. The verb in Zulu. Johannesburg, Witwatersrand University Press. BENDER,‘ LIONEL M. and Hailu Fulas. 1978. Amharic Verb Morphology. Language and Linguistics, 1. Monograph 7. East Lansing. MI. African Studies Center. Michigan State University. BENDIX, EDWARD HERMAN. 1966. Componential analysis of general vocabulary: The Hague: Mouton. 308 BENNETT PATRIC R. 1969. A comparative study of four Thagiau Verbal systems: The inflectional systems of Kikuyu Kamba Embu, and Mwimbi. Ph.D. Thesis. School of Oriental and. African Studies. University' of Lonson. 1982. The status of predicator in Dhaagicw and Bantu. Etudes Linguistiques Vol. 3 1982. The status of the predicator in Dhaagicw and Bantu. Etudes Linguistiques Vol. 3. BERGVAL VICTORIA. 1987. A typology of empty categories for Kikuyu and Swahili. Current approaches to African linguistics, ed. by Gerrit J. Dimmendaal, 3 Dordrecht: Foris, 198x. BETBEDER, PAUL W. F. 1943. Empondera Yezu ((Haya): Followers of Jesus). Vic. Apost. De Bukoba. Kashozi. and JOHN JONES. 1949. A handbook of the Haya Language. White Fathers Printing Press Bukoba. Tanganyika territory (Tanzania). BLOOM, et al. 1980. Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language 56.386- 412. BLOOMFIELD, LEONARD.1933. The structure of learned words. The structure of language, ed. by Charles F. Hockett 1970:252-256. BOAS, HANS U. 1974. On Halle’s "Prolegomena to a theory of word formation" or what is a linguistic generalization? Linguistics 134:5-8. ‘ BOKAMBA, EYAMBA. 1985. Verbal agreement as a noncyclic rule. In Bantu African Linguistics. Studies in the sciences of language series 6:9-54. BOLINGER, DWIGHT L. 1950. Rime, assonance and morpheme analysis. In Bolinger 1965:203-226. 1965a Forms‘ of English, Accent, Morpheme, Order. Cambridge Massachusetts. Harvard University Press. 1965b. On defining the morpheme. Reprinted in Bolinger I). 1“ Forms of English; Accent, Morpheme, Order. Cambridge, Harvard University Press, 183-89. BONA BAISI, JOSEPH (1957). IkanVI—Ngambo (Haya dictionary)‘ Rwamishenye, Bukoba Pallotine Fathers Hamburg Lahn (Germany) Printing Press. 309 BOOIJ, GEERT. 1977. Dutch Morphology. A study of word formation in Generative Grammar. The Peter de Ridder Presse, Lisse. 1979. Semantic regularities 111 word. formation. Linguistics 17, 985-1001. BOTNE, ROBERT DALE OLSON. 1980. Phonological and morphological aspects of verbal ‘ structures in Kinyarwanda. M. A. Thesis, Northwestern University. 1981. On the nature of tense and aspect. Studies in the semantics of temporal reference in English and Kinyarwanda. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University. Studies in African Linguistics December 1986, 17:3:317. 1983. The semantics of tense in Kinyarwanda. Studies in African Linguistics 14:3:235-265. BRESNAN, JOAN and SAM A. MCHOMBO. 1987. Topic, Pronoun, and Agreement in Chichewa. Language 63.4:741-782. 1982. Control and Complementation. LI 13.343-434. BROWN, GILIAN and G. YULE. 1983. Discourse analysis. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. BRYAN, MARGARET A. 1959. The Bantu languages of Africa. London. Oxford University Press. BUKOBA DIOCESE. 1987. Rumuli ("The torch"). A Haya Newspaper V010 10-150 BUREAU OF STATISTICS. 1982. 1978 Population CenSus Volume IV: A summary of selected statsitics. Ministry of Planning and Economic Affairs. Dar-es-Salaam. BYARUSHENGO, ERNEST RUGWA. 1975. An Examination of the Segmental Phonology of Haya. Unpublished M. A. Thesis, University of Dar-es-Salaam. and SARAH TENENBAUM. 1976a. Agreement and word order: A case for pragmatics in Haya. Berkely Linguistic Society 2.89-99. et al. 1976b. Tone, Accent, and Assertion. Haya Studies in Bantu tonology, ed. by L. Hyman. Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics 3: 183-205. et al. 1977. Haya Grammatical Structure: Phonology, Grammar, Discourse. Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics No. 6. University of California Los Angeles, California. 310 BYBEE, JOAN 'L. 1985. Morphology: a study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam, Philadelphia. John Benjamin Publishing Company. and C. In MODER. 1983. Morphological Classes as natural categories. Language. 59.251-270. CANNON, GERALD. 1986. "Blends in English word formation." Linguistics: 24.4:725-754. CARLSON, GREG and T . ROEPER. 1979 . Morphology and subcategorization case auui the unmarked complex verb, Lexical grammar ed. by T. Hoekstra and M. Mootgat Glot 3/4:123-64. CARSTAIRS, ANDREW D. 1983. "Paradigm economy." Journal of Linguistics 19:115-125. 1984» .Constraints (n1 allomorphy 1J1 inflection: Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistic Club. CHAFE, WALLACE L. 1970. Meaning and the structure or language. The University of Chicago Press. Chicago. CHAGAS, JEREMY E. 1977. The preprefix. Byarushengo, E. (ed.1 et al. Haya Grammatical Structure. SCOPIL 6.35-44. University' of Southern California” Los Angeles, California. 1977. The preprefix. Haya grammatical structure, ed. by Byarushengo E. et al. SCOPIL 6:35-44. CHOMSKY, NOAM. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. MA: MIT Press. and MORRIS HALLE. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row. 1970. Remarks on Nominalization. A. Jacobs and P. S. Rosembaun (eds). 1972 Studies on Semantics in Generative Grammar. The Hague: Mouton. 1984. Lectures on Government and Binding. The Pisa Lectures. Foris Publications. Holland/Cinnaminson UOSOAO CLARK, EVE V. and HERBERT H. CLARK. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55:767-811. 311 CLARK, M. 1978. A dynamic treatment of tone with special attention to the tonal system of Igbo. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Massachusetts. CLEMENS, GEORGE N. 1984. "Principles of Tone assignment in Kikuyu." Clemens, G. N. and J. Goldsmith (eds). Autosegmental Studies in Bantu. Tone. Foris Publications. Dordrecht-Holland/Cinnaminson. U.S.A. and JOHN, GOLDSMITH. 1984. Autosegmental studies in Bantu Tone. Publications in African languages and linguistics. Foris Publications. Dordrecht- Holland/Cinnaminson. U.S.A. COLE DESMOND T. 1955. An introduction to Tswana Grammar. London. Longmans, Green and Co. COMRIE, BERNARD. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge. London University Press. Cambridge. 1981. Language universals and linguistic typology: syntax and morphology. Oxford: Blackwell. 3. 1985. Tense. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge: London. CONTINI, ELLEN MORAVA. 1983. Tense and non tense in Swahili Grammar. Semantic asymmetry between affirmative and negative (Bantu, Africa). Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. Columbia University. COWIE, A. P. 1981. The treatment of collocations and idioms in learners dictionary. Applied Linguistics 2:223-35. 1982. Polysemy and the structure of lexical fields. Nottingham Linguistic Circular. 11.2:51-64. CRUSE, D. A. 1982. On lexical ambiguity. Nottingham Linguistics Circular. 11.2:65-80. 1986. Lexical Semantics. London, ’Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. CUTLER, ANNE. 1980. Productivity in word formation. Papers from the 16‘! Regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago. 45-51. 1981. Degrees of transparency in word formation. The Canadian journal of Linguistics 26:73-77. DAHL, OSTEN. 1984. Temporal distance: remoteness distinctions in tense-aspect systems, Explanations for language universals, ed by B. Butterworth, B. Comrie and O. Dahl. The Hague: Mouton. 312 DALGISH, GERALD M. 1977. "Past tense formation in OruHaya" African Language/Language Africaines 3:78-92. 1978. The syntax and semantics of morpheme NI in Kivunjo (Chaga). Kiswahili, Journal of the Institute of Kiswahili Research. ed. by A. G. Gibbe, Pp. 42-46. University of Dar-es-Sallam. DE BLOIS, KEES. 1970. The augment in the Bantu languages. Africana Linguistics IV, 85-165 Tervuren. DELL, F., and E. O. SELKIRK. 1978. On a Morphologically Governed Vowel Alternation in French, Recent Transformation Studies in European Linguistics, ed. by S. J. Keyser, Linguistic Inquiry' Monograph 3, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DI SEIULLO, ANNA-MARIA. 1987. On the definition of Word. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DIK, SIMON C. 1967. Some critical remarks on the treatment of morphological structure in transformational generative grammar. Lingua 18: 352-83. DOKE, CLEMENT M. 1927. Textbook of Zulu Grammar. Longmans Southern Africa (PTY) LTD. Cape Town. 1943. Outline grammar of Bantu. Mimeo. 1945. Bantu; modern grammatical, phonetical and lexicographical studies since 1860. London. Dawnsons of Pall Mall. 1954. The Southern Bantu Languages. London. Longmans, Green and Co. and MOFOKENG, S. M. 1957. Textbook of Southern Sotho Grammar. London. Longmans, Green and Co. DRESSLER, WOLFGANG, U. 1985. Morphophonology. The dynamics of derivation. Ann Arbor. Karoma Press. DRIEVER, DOROTHEA. 1975. Aspects of the case of grammar of Mombasa Swahiii. Hamberger Philogische Studien 43. DUBNOVE, ELLEN ZINOV’EVNA. 1984. The Rwanda Language. Nauka Publishers. Central Department of Oriental literature. Moscow. DURANTI, A. 1979. Object clitic pronouns in Bantu and the topicality hierarchy. Studies in African Linguistics 10.1:31-46. 313 EASTMAN, CAROL M. 1969a. A descriptive statement of operative verbal extension in Swahili: African Language Studies, X, SOAS. 1969b. The status of the reversive extension in modern Kenya Coastal Swahili. Journal of African Linguistics 8.29-39. FARNSWHORTH, E. M. 1954. A Kamba Grammar. Cyclostyled, AIM. FLEICHMAN, SUZANNE. 1977. Culture and Linguistic factors in word formation. University of California Publications in Linguistics. Berkley and Los Angeles. University of California Press. FORTUNE, G. S. J. 1955. An analytical Grammar of Shona. London. Longmans, Green and Co. FREIDEN, ROBERT. 1975. The analysis of passives; Language. FROMKIN, VICTORIA A. 1978. Tone: a linguistic survey. New York: Academic Press Inc. GARDNER, JEAN LOUIS. 1985. Relational Grammar. The Catholic University of America Press. Washington D.C. GARY, JUDITH 0. AND EDWARD L. KEENAN. 1977. "On Collapsing Grammatical Relations in Universal Grammar." Syntax and Semantics: Relational Grammar, ed. by Cole, P. and J. M. Sadock. New York. Academic Press. 85-120. GAZDAR, GERALD. 6H: al. 1985. Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar. Harvard University Press. Cambridge, Massachusetts. GIVON,.TALMY. 1972. Studies in ChiBemba and Bantu grammar. Studies in African Linguistics. Supplement 3:174-179 and 203-216. 1976. Topic, pronoun, and grammatical agreement. LI, CHARLES N (ed.1 Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press. GOLDSMITH, JOHN. 1976. Auto segmental phonology. Unpublished MIT Ph.D. dissertation, reproduced by Indiana University Linguistics Club. GREENBERG, JOSEPH. H. 1954. A quantitative approach to morphological typology of language. IJAL 26.178-194. 314 1963a. Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements . Universals of Human Language. Greenberg J. H. 73-113. Cambridge, MA. MIT Press. 1963b. Universals of Language. 2nd ed. Cambridge, MazMIT Press. GRICE, PAUL H. 1975. Logic and Conversation. Speech acts (Syntax and Semantics,3), eds. by Peter Cole and Jerry L. Morgan, 41-58. New York: Academic Press. GUNTER, RICHARD. 1972. English derivation. Journal of Linguistics 8.1-19. GUTHRIE, MALCOLM 1948. The classification of Bantu languages. London. Oxford University Press. 1967. Comparative Bantu I-IV Farnborough: Gregg Press. 1971-74. Comparative Bantu. Farnborough: Gregg Press. GUTT, ERNST AUGUST. 1985. A multi-tiered to silt’i verb morphology. Studies 1J1 African Linguistics. 16.2.183- 222. HALLE, MORRIS. 1973. Prolegomena to a theory of word formation“ Linguistic inquiry, 4.1:3-16. HARTMANN, RENE. (ed.1. 1983. Lexicography: principles and practice. London and New York Academic Press. HAWKINSON, ANNE and LARRY M. HYMAN. 1974. Hierarchies of natural toch in Shona Studies in African Linguistics 5: 147-170. HEDINGERN ROBERT. 1985. The Verb in .Akoose. Studies in African Linguistics. 16.1.1-56. HEINE, BERND. and ULRIKE CLAUDI. 1986. On the rise of grammatical categories. Berlin Dietrich Reimer Verlag. HENY, FRANK and B. RICHARDS. (eds.) 1983 Linguistic categories: Auxiliaries and related puzzles. Vol. 1: Categories, Vol. 2: The scope, order and distribution of English auxiliary verbs. Syntheses language library 19 and 20 Dordrecht: Reidel. HOBLEY, JUNE. 1965. Bassa verbal formations. Journal of West African languages. 11:2.39-50. 315 HOCKETT, CHARLES. 1947. Problems of morphemic analysis. Language 23:321-43. HOOPER, JOAN B. 1976A. Word frequency in lexical diffusion and source (fl? morphophonological change. Current progress in historical linguistics, ed. by W. M. Christie. Amsterdam North Holland 95-105. 1976B. An introduction to natural generative phonology. New York: Academic Press. HUDSON, GROVER. 1979. Is deep structure linear? Linear order and Generative theory. ed. by Meisel, J. M. and M. D. Pam. Philadelphia John Benjamins B. V. 1982. Review of Amharic Verb Morphology. M. L. Bender and Hailu Fulas. Afroasiatic Linguistics 8, 4:3- 8. 1984. The Arabic double verb conspiracy and morpheme invariance. Paper presented at the 15th Conference on African Linguistics. Los Angeles. 1986. Arabic root and pattern morphology without tiers. Journal of Linguistics 22:85-122 printed in Great Britain University Press. HUDSON, RICHARD. 1984. Word Grammar. New York. Basil Blackwell Ltd. HUTCHISON, JOHN P. 1981 The Kanuri: a reference grammar University of Wisconsin. HYMAN, LARY. 1976. Studies in Bantu tonology. SCOPIL 3. University of Southern California. Los Angeles. and A. DURANTI. 1982. On the object Relationship in Bantu. Syntax and Semantics: Studies in Transitivity. Ed. by Hopper, P. J. and S. A. Thomson. 15.217-259. and E . R . BYARUSHENGO . 1 984 . A model of Haya tonology. Autosegmental studies in Bantu tone, Ed. by G. M. Clemens and J. Goldsmith 3.53-104. ISHUMI, ABEL G. M. 1980. Kiziba: The cultural heritage of an Old African Kingdom. Syracuse University. JACKENDOFF, RAY. 1975. Morphological and semantic regularities in the lexicon, Language. 51:639-71. JANSEN, THEO J. and M. STRONG-JENSEN. 1984. Morphology is in the Lexicon! Linguistic Inquiry 15:474-98. 316 JOHNSON, MARK. 1980. A semantic description of temporal reference in Kikuyu verb. Studies in African Linguistics 11:269-320. JOOS, MARTIN. 1957. Readings in linguistics I. Chicago. The University of Chicago Press. JUILLAND, ALPHONSE and P. M. EDWARDS. 1971. The Rumanian Verb system. The Hague: Mouton. 1978. Transformational and structural morphology. About two rival approaches to the Rumanian Verb System. Anma Libri and Co. KASTOVSKY, D. 1977. Word formation, or: at the crossroads of morphology, syntax, semantics and Lexicon. Folia Linguistica 10:1-33. KHAMISI, ABDU M. 1983. The stative derivational affix in Swahili. Working Papers in Lingu1stics, Dept of Linguistics University of Hawaii Vol. 15, 2:93-113. KIDIMA, LUKOWA. 1987. Object agreement and topicality hierarchies. Studies in African linguistics 18.2:175- 208. KIEFER, FERENC. 1972. A propos derivational morphology. Kiefer F. (ed.) Derivational processes. Stockholm, KVAL, 42-59. KIMENYI , ALEXANDRE . 1979 . Studies in Kinyarwanda . and Bantu phonology. Carbondale (U.S.A.) and Edmonton (Canada). Linguistic Research, Inc. KIPARSHY, PAUL. 1982. Lexical morphology and phonology. In Linguistics in the morning calm, ed. by I. S. Yang. Seoul: Hanslin 3-91. KISSEBERTH, CHARLES W. and M. I. ABASHEIKH. 1977. Object Relationship in Chi-mwizni, a Bantu Language. Syntax and Semantics: Grammatical Relations, ed. by Cole, P. and J. M. Sadock. Academic Press. New York. 179-218. KOOPMAN, HILDA. 1984. The syntax of verbs: From -Verb Movement Rules in the Kru Languages to Universal Grammar. Dordrecht-Holland/Cinnaminson- U.S.A. Foris Publications. KOUTSOUDAS, ,ANDREAS. 1972. ’The strict order Fallacy.’ Language 48:88-96. 1981. Constraints on linearization rules. Language 57.4:818-840. 317 KURYLOWICZ, JERZY. 1964. The inflectional categories of Indo European. Heidelberg, C. Winter. LACTOT, A. 1959. Evangeli entakatifu nebikorwa byentumwa ((Haya) ("The holly bible and the work of the angels")). Marianum Press. Kisubi, Uganda. Marianum Press. LADEFOGED, PETER et al. 1972. Language in Uganda. London. Oxford University Press. LAPOINTE, STEBENG. 1983. A comparison of two recent theories of agreement. Papers from the parasession on the interplay of phonology, morphology, and syntax. Chicago Linguistics Society. Chicago. 122-134. LI, CHARLES N. (ed.). 1975. Word order and. word order change. Austin: University of Texas Press. LIEBER, ROCHELLE. 1980. On the organization of the lexicon. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. LIGHTER, THEODORE M. 1975. The role of derivational morphology in generative grammar. Language 51.617-38. LWAMGIRA, F. X. 1949. Amakuru ga Kiziba na Abakma bamu (History of Kiziba and its chiefs). Rwamishenye, Bukoba Printers. LYONS, JOHN. 1968. Introduction to theoretical linguistics: New York: Cambridge University Press. 1977. Semantics. Vol 1 and 2. New York: Cambridge University Press. MACHOBANE, MALILO. 19XX Tense and Aspect in Sesotho. Bloomington. Indiana University Linguistics Club. MAGIER, DAVID. 1985. Temporal reference points and the semantics of framing. Evidence from Marwari. Presented at the 60‘“ meeting of the linguistic society of America. Seattle. MALKIEL, YAKOV. 1978. Derivational categories. Universals of human language, ed. by J. H. Greenberg. Vol. 3 Word- structure. Stanford. Stanford University Press 125-49. MANCZAK, WITOLD. 1980. Laws of analogy. Historical morphology. ed. by J. Fisiak. 283-6. The Hague: MoUton. MARLE, J. V. 1985. On the paradigmatic dimension of morphological creativity. Dordrecht-Holland/Cinnaminson U.S.A. Foris publications. 318 MASAMBA, DAVID. P. B. 1976. Kiambisho-Awali (Pre-prefix) Katika Kiruri (Pre-prefix in Kiruri). KISWAHILI. Journal of the Institute of Kiswahili Research). 46/2: 23-34 0 MATHEWS, PETER H. 1970. Recent development in morphology. New horizons in Linguistics ed. by Lyons J. 96-114. Harmondsworth, Baltimore, Penguin Books. 1972. Inflectional morphology. A theoretical study based on aspects of Latin Verb Conjugation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1974. Morphology: an introduction to the theory of word-structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. MAW, JOAN. 1985. Twende! A practical Swahili Course. London. Oxford University Press. MCCARTHY, JAMES D. 1979. Formal problems in semantic, phonology and morphology. Doctoral dissertation, Cambridge MAzMIT Press. MEEUSSEN, ACHIEL EMIEL. 1976. Bantu grammatical reconstructions, Africana Linguistics III:79-128. MORRIS, HENRY FRANCIS and B. E. R. KIRWAN. 1957. A Runyankore grammar. The Eagle Press. East African Literature Bureau. Kampala. Uganda. MOODY, MARVIN D. 1978. Some preliminaries to a theory of morphology, Glossa, 12:16-38. MORSE, MARY LYNN ALICE. 1976. A sketch of the phonology and morphology' of Bobo (Upper 'Volta). Unpublished. Ph.D. dissertation. Columbia University. MOSHI, LIOBA. 1986. "Functional typology of "hi" in Kivunjo." A paper presented at the 17th African Linguistic Conference at Indiana University, April 2.5. 1988. Tense, aspect and other time markers "in Kivunjo (Chaga). A peper presented at the 19th African Linguistics Conference. Boston University. MOULD, MARTIN. 1976. Comparative Grammar Reconstruction and Language Subclassification: the North Victoria Languages. Ph.D. Dissertation, U.C.L.A. MUJWAHUZI, BERNARD. 1950. Obugorozi bwa Buhaya ((Haya). ("The leadership of Buhaya"). Bukoba. Tanzania. NEWMAN, STANLEY S. 1948. English suffixation: a descriptive approach. WORD 4:24-36. 319 NIDA, EUGENE A. 1946. Morphology. The descriptive analysis of words. Ann Arbor. University of Michigan Press, 2“‘1 ed. 1948 . The identification of morphemes . Reprinted in Readings in Linguistics 1. ed. by Joos. The University of Chicago. 1957:255-271. NURSE, DEREK. 1979. Description of sample Bantu languages of Tanzania. African languages/Langues Africaines 5.1. London: International African Institute. NYMAN, M. 1987. Is the paradigm economy relevant? Journal of Linguistics 23:251-262. ODENDAL, F. F. 1963. Limitations of morphological processes. A note. Lingua 12:220-225. OKUNOR, V. 1967. Tone in the GA verb. Institute of African Studies. Accra. University of Ghana. PENNANEN, E. V. 1972. Current views of word-formation. Newphilologische Mitteilungen 73:292-308. PEREZ, CAROLYN H. 1986. Aspects of Complementation in three Bantu Languages. Indiana University Linguistics Club. 1987. Successive cyclic movement in Kitharaka. A paper presented at the 18th African Linguistic conference in Montreal Canada. PERKINS, REVERE D. 1980. The evolution of culture and grammar. SUNY/Buffalo. Ph.D. Dissertation. PESETSKY, DAVID. 1985 . Morphology 'and logical form. Linguistic inquiry Vol. 16.2:193-246. PHILLIPS, JUDITH W. 1983. A partial grammar of the Haitian creole verb system: forms, functions and syntax. SUNY/Buffalo Ph.D. Dissertation. PLATT , D . 198 1 . The balance between regularity and irregularity in verbal inflection. Inflectional morphology: Introduction to the extended word-and- paradigm theory , ed . by T . Thomas Flinders . U . C . L . A Occasional papers, 4:41-71. POLOME, EDGAR C. 1976. Swahili language Handbook. Washington D.C. Center for applied Linguistics. PORT, ROBERT F. 1982. Morphophonemics of Swahili Verb suffixes. Studies in African Linguistics. 13:3:248-271. 320 RARDIN, R. B. 1975. Studies in derivational morphology. Ph.D. dissertation MIT. RASCHER, ANNA. 1958. Guide for learning the Ruhaya Language. Bielefeld, Bethel Mission. RAY, SIDNEY H. 1933. A grammar of Kiwai language, Fly Delta, with a Kiwai vocabulary by E. B. Riley. Port Moresby, Australia: Goverment Printer. REICHENBACH, HANS. 1947. The tenses of verbs. Section 51. Elements of symbolic logic. McMillan. REYNODS, KARL. 1985. Swahili Morphology. Manuscript. ROEPER‘, THOMAS and MUFFY E. A. SIEGEL. 1978. A lexical transformation for Verbal Compounds. Linguistics inquiry 9.199-260. RUBANZA , YUNUS I . 1979 . The relationship between Ki swahili and other African languages: the case of Kihaya. Unpublished M.A. thesis. University of Dar-es-Salaam. 1988. Restrictions in the linear ordering of the Haya verb morphemes. A paper presented at the 19th African Linguistic Conference. Boston University. SABIMANA, FIRMARD. 1986. The relational structure of the Kirundi Verb. (Burundi) Unpublished Ph.D dissertation. Indiana University Club. SALONE, SUKARI. 1977. Conditionals. Haya grammatical structure ed. E. Byarushengo et al. 149-159. California, University of Southern Carolina. 1979. Typology of conditionals and conditionals in Haya. Studies in African Linguistics. 10.1:65-80. SAMPSON, GEOFFREY. 1979. The indivisibility of words. Journal of Linguistics 15.39-47. SANDERS, GEORGE. 1970. Constraints on constituent. ordering. Papers in Linguistics 2.460-502. 1975a. Invariant ordering. The Hague: Mouton. 1975b. "On the explanation of constituent order universals." C. N. LI 389-436. SCALISE, SERGIO. 1986. Generative. Morphology. Foris Publications. Dordrecht-Holland/Cinnaminson. U.S.A. (Second Edition) 321 SCHADEBERG. THILD C. 1984. A sketch of Swahili. Morphology. Dordrecht. Holland/Cinnaminson. U.S.A. Foris Publications. ' SCHRAMM, GENE M. 1962. An outline of classical Arabic verb structure. Language 38:360-75. SCOTTON , CAROL MYERS . 1 96 7a . Semantic and syntact ic subcategorization in Swahili causative verb shapes. Journal of African Linguistics 6:249-267. 1967b. Aspects of the Swahili extended verb system with special reference to some deep structure syntactic and semantic restrictions. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation. University of Wisconsin. Madison. SEITEL, PETER. 1970’s-1980’s. A collection of Haya Epic. Mimeo. 1987. Haya epic and heroic poetry. Mimeo. SELKIRK, ELISABETH O. 1982. The syntax of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. . SIEGEL, DOROTHY. 1974. Topics in English morphology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 1977. The adjacency condition and the theory of morphology. Proceedings of NELS VIII, ed. by Mark, J. Stein. 189-197. SIMOES, MARIA C. P. and CAROL STOEL-GAMMON. 1979. The acquisition of inflections in Portuguese: a study of the development of person markers on verbs. Journal of child language 6.53-67. SINGLER, JOHN VICTOR. 1985. The status of lexical association and the obligatory contour principle in the analysis of tone languages. African Linguistics. SSLS 6:491-507. SKOUSEN, ROYAL. 1974. An explanation theory of morphology. Chicago Linguistic Society. 318-27. STEPHANY, URSULA. 1981. Verbal Grammar in Modern Greek early child language. P. Dale and D. Ingram (eds) Child language: an international perspective. Baltimore: University Park Press. ' STRAUSS, STEVEN L. 1980. How abstract is English morphology? Glossa 14:89-112. 322 STUMP, GREGORY T. 1984. Agreement vs. inflection in Breton. Natural language and Linguistic Theory 2.289-348. TALMY, LEONARD. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms. Language typology and syntactic description, III: Grammatical categories and lexicon, ed. by Timothy Shopen, 57-149 Cambridge: University Press. TAYLOR, CHARLES. 1985. Croom Helm Descriptive Grammars: Nkore-Kiga. Croom Helm. London. THOMAS-FLINDERS, TRACY. 1981. Evidence for inflection as a phonological process. Inflectional morphology: Introduction to the extended word-and-paradigm theory ed. by T. Thomas-Flinders. U.C.L.A. Occasional papers. 4:72-83. TROMMELEN, MIEKE. 1986. Dutch Morphology: Evidence for the right hand head rule. Linguistic inquiry 17.1:147-170. UBELS, EDWARD H. 1983. Mood and aspect in Karang. Studies in African Linguistic 14.1:47-70. ULTAN, R. 1978. The nature of future tenses. Universals of human language, ed. by J. Greenberg. California. Stanford University Press. 3:81083. VAIL, HAZEN LEROY. 1972. Aspects of the Tumbaku Verg. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation University of Wisconsin. Madison. VENDLER, ZENO. 1967. Verbs and times. Chapter 4 of Z. Vendler. Linguistics in Philosophy. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press. 97-121. VENNEMANN , THEO . 1 97 3 . Explanation in syntax . J . Kimball (ed.). Syntax and. Semantics II. New ‘York: Academic Press. ' , VINCENT , NIGEL . 1 979 . Word order and grammatical theory . Linear Order and Generative Theory, ed. by Meisel, J. M. and M. D. Pam. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. VITALE, ANTHONY J. 1981. Swahili Syntax. Foris Publications Cinnaminson. U.S.A. VOEGELIN, C. F. and F. M. VOEGELIN. 1966. Index to the languaes of the world . Anthropological linguistics ,‘ 6 and 7. WALKER, D. 1986. Review. Morphology: A study of the relation between. meaning and form, by’ Bybee, J. Journal of linguistics 22:493-496. 323 WELMERS, WILLIAM E. 1970. The derivation of Igbo verb bases. Studies in African Linguistics 1:49-59. 1973. African Language Structures. Berkley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. WHEELER, CATHY J. & SCHUMSKY, DONALD A. 1980. The morpheme boundaries of some English derivational suffixes. Glossa. 14:3-340 WHORF, BENJAMIN LEE. 1936. The punctual and segmentative aspects of verbs in Hopi. Language 12:127-31. 1950. An American Indian model of the universe. International Journal of American Linguistics, 16. WILLIAMS, EDWINS. 1981a. On the notions "lexically related" and ’head of a word.’ Linguistic Inquiry 12:245-274. 1981b. Argument structure and morphology. Linguistic review 1:81-114. WILT, TIMOTHY L. 1988. Bukavu Swahili. A sociolinguistic study of language change. Ph.D. Dissertation Department of Linguistics: Michigan State University. YULE, GEORGE. 1985. The Study of Language. An introduction: Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. ZAWAWI, SHABRIFA M. 1979. Loan words and their effect on the classification of Swahili nominals. E. J. Brill, Leiden: The Netherlands. ZIMMER, KARL E. 1964. Affixical negation in English and other languages . Supplement to Word 20 . 2 Monograph 5 . N.Y. Linguistic circle of New York. ZWANEMBURG, W. 1980. Form and meaning in morphology. Quaderni di Semantica. 2:327-38. ZWICKY, ARNOLD M. and R. E. WALLACE (eds.) 1984. Working papers in linguistics. 29. The Ohio State University. Papers on Morphology.