II I I III I II II I \ THE EFFECT OF SHORT-TERM GROUP COUNSELING AND COGNITIVE LEARNING ON A MEASURE 0F SELF -ACTUALIZATION 0F COUNSELORS IN TRAINING Thesis fat the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY WAYNE ROWE 1971 R 2434 I 7 WI I LIBRAR Y Michigan State University IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 31293 00642 713 This is to certify that the thesis entitled Actualization of Counselors in Traininq presented by The bffect of Short-term Group Counneling and Connitive Learning on a Reasure of Self- Hayne Rowe has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph,12, degree in Cgunseli ng Date July 504 197L I 0-7539 - JN‘Sme av ' ~ IIUAG 3 SIINS' . EIW'INL‘: LIBRARY emosns .mppmcmm ABSTRACT THE EFFECT OF SHORT-TERM GROUP COUNSELING AND COGNITIVE LEARNING ON A MEASURE OF SELF-ACTUALIZATION OF COUNSELORS IN TRAINING BY Wayne Rowe The primary purpose of this study was to assess the effect which short-term group counseling and a cogni- tive learning experience may have on the Personal Orien- tation Inventory (POI), a measure of self-actualization. It was suggested that influences other than those usually attributed to group counseling may account in part for changes previously found on devices of this sort. Seventy-two counselor candidates were assigned to the following treatments: group counseling, leader theory consistent with self-actualization; group counsel- ing, leader theory based on social-learning; seminar pre- senting self-actualization concepts in a favorable manner; placebo treatment; inactive control. Treatment was admin- istered 14 hours during 8 sessions. Pre- and post-testing was carried out with the POI and Carkhuff's Discrimination Test. Wayne Rowe Pre-test scores were used as covariates in a series of multivariate and univariate analyses of covari- ance. Treatment effects could not be demonstrated at the .05 level of significance using any of the scales which were employed. An analysis of possible factors contributing to the obtained results included issues related to: theory, sampling, design and statistical treatment, measurement, and presentation and control of treatments. THE EFFECT OF SHORT-TERM GROUP COUNSELING AND COGNITIVE LEARNING ON A MEASURE OF SELF-ACTUALIZATION OF COUNSELORS IN TRAINING BY Wayne Rowe A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1971 TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES I O O O O O C O O 0 Chapter I. II. III. IV: THE PROBLEM, RATIONALE, AND RELATED RESEARCH . . . . . Need . . . . . . . . . . Purpose . . . . . . . . . Theory . . . . . . . . . Review of the Literature Hypotheses . . . . . . . Overview . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY 0 O O C O O I 0 Subjects . . . . . . . . Treatments . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . Instruments . . . . . . . Testable Hypotheses . . . Design and Statistical Analysis . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . ANALYSIS OF RESULTS . . . . Preliminary Data . . . . Treatment Effects . . . . Non-Experimental Data . . Summary . . . . . . . . . SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION . . Summary . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . Conclusion . . . . . . . ‘;iii Page UlobU'IOJNH |-’ P4P 16 17 27 27 31 32 34 35 35 36 39 41 43 43 45 51 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . APPENDICES Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Typescript of Leader- Member Interactions Instructions to Judges Activity Evaluation Form Table A.1 Mean POI Sub- scale Scores Table A.2 Mean, Variance, and Standard Deviation of All Variables iv Page 53 S7 57 60 61 62 65 Table 2.1 3.2 3.3 3.5 A.1 LIST OF TABLES Test for Difference in Leader Responses . . . . . . . . . . . . Graphic Representation of the Research Design . . . . . . . . . Summary of Pre- and Post-test Scores of Major Interest . . . . Multivariate Test of Hypothesis 1 POI Scores . . . . . . . . . . . Overall ANCOVA Analysis of Major POI scales 0 O O O O O O O O O 0 Overall ANCOVA Analysis of Discrimination Test . . . . . . . Test for Pre- and Post-test Differences . . . . . . . . . . . Mean POI Sub-scale Scores . . . . Mean, Variance, and Standard Deviation of All Variables . . . Page 26 33 36 37 38 38 39 62 65 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM, RATIONALE, AND RELATED RESEARCH Need The recommendation that counselor trainees be pro- vided with preparation and experience in group counseling has been set forth in both the policy statement of the American Personnel and Guidance Association (1961) and the position on standards for counselor preparation of the Association for Counselor Education and Supervision (1967). However, the particular value of such experience has not been explicitly stated. Purposes could reasonably be ad- vanced which would range from strictly didactic goals, on one hand, to "deep" therapeutic objectives, on the other. In their comprehensive appraisal of group counsel- ing research, Gazda and Larsen (1968) conclude that "group counseling research is inconclusive (p. 64)." Yet many counselor training programs seem to take for granted that participation in group counseling will result in benefi- cial personality changes among counselors-in-training. A commonly held position is that participation in group counseling enhances the potential of the prospective counselor because of the type of presumed personality change likely to take place as a result of the group experience. Proponents of this general View have emphasized such points as emotional growth (Wirt, Betz, and Engle, 1969), personal growth (Hurst and Jensen, 1968), experiencing real self (McLain, 1969), and self-understanding (McKinnon, 1969). Support for this position is seen in research which has purported to assess such alterations through the use of a variety of personality instruments such as the Edwards Per- sonal Preference Schedule, parts of the Minnesota Multipha- sic Personality Inventory, self-concept Q-sorts, various projective devices, and the Personal Orientation Inventory. However, since changes in behavior are not measured directly by these instruments, it is not clear whether score changes on such devices can be attributed solely to the therapeutic value of group counseling or if cognitive pro- cesses keyed to the implicit model of psychological adjust- ment may contribute to the measured effect. Therefore, there is a need to compare the effects associated with group counseling and a cognitive presentation on a typical person- ality instrument and other measures of relevance to the field of counseling. PUE BOSE The primary purpose of the study was to assess the effect upon counselor trainees which three kinds of learning experience may have on a measure of self-actualization. In addition, the effect of these experiences upon a mea- sure of ability to discriminate counselor responses of varied therapeutic utility was investigated. The experiences which were provided included par- ticipation in group counseling presented by leaders of two different theoretical orientations and participation in a lecture-discussion group. In one treatment the group leader had an orientation consistent with the concept of self-actualization. In another treatment the group leader had an orientation which was not consistent with the con— cept of self-actualization. In the third treatment, the content of the lecture-discussion sessions was chosen to present the concept of self-actualization in a favorable manner. The objective was to determine if any of these experiences can be demonstrated to produce a significant effect on the measures employed when administered to sub- jects who are counselors-in-training. Theory Fundamental to the notion that participation in group counseling has therapeutic utility in counselor ed- ucation programs is the premise that counseling effective- ness increases as the counselor more closely approximates an "ideal" state of psychological well-being. Terms used by theorists to indicate this model condition include fully- functioning (Rogers, 1963), authentic (Jourard, 1966), self- actualized (Maslow, 1967), and'whole (Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967). In each case the effectiveness of the counselor is thought to be affected positively to the extent that his personality incorporates attributes of the construct which may be used. Counselor preparation, from this point of View, ought to include some attempt to foster and guide the "per- sonal growth" of the prospective counselor because of the presumed relationship between psychological well-being or fulfillment (approximation to the model) and counseling ca- pability. Particular aspects of "personal growth" which have been cited as having potential beneficial effects for prospective counselors include: self— and other-acceptance, empathic understanding, and inter-personal sensitivity (Hurst and Jensen, 1968); self-knowledge and perceptive skills (Seegars and McDonald, 1963); self concept develOp- ment (Wirt, Betz, and Engle, 1969); self-understanding (Anderson and Cabianca, 1970; Bonney and Gazda, 1966; McKinnon, 1969). Group counseling, from this point of view, has as its major purpose the alteration of the self along these or other similar dimensions. While other more limited or spe- cific purposes for group counseling have been advanced (Krumboltz, 1968; Muro, 1968; Orton, 1965; Zimpfer, 1968), proponents of this point of View emphasize affective devel- opment, rather than cognitive learning focused upon group dynamics, leader roles, particular techniques, and the like. At issue is the question of whether changes of a therapeutic nature can be attributed to the kinds of evidence which has been gathered on counselor trainees. Review of the Literature It is generally agreed that group counseling exper- ience is often accepted as an element in counselor educa- tion programs in spite of little experimental evidence con- cerning the value of such activities (Betz, 1969; Wirt, Betz, and Engle, 1969). In a brief analysis of the litera- ture, Betz (1969) identified several limitations, some of which include the following: (a) the literature is largely descriptive; (b) counselor educators and trainees evaluate group experiences positively; and (c) the measure of outcome employed in the few experimental studies reported is usually a pencil-paper device--an indirect measure of performance. This condition is unfortunate because any situation in which theoretical investments are heavy and tangible evidence is sparse may not be a climate that is hospitable to reason. Given (a) and (b), above, it is difficult to accept non- experimental evaluations of group counseling effects. In addition, the interpretation of data under (c) must be care- fully examined. The survey reported below represents a rather comprehensive review of the literature on group coun- seling with counselor trainees. Other reports of particular relevance are also included. Several studies have attempted to determine whether participation in group counseling did, in fact, bring about some positive change in the personality structure of coun- selor trainees. Gazda and Ohlsen (1961) reported on 34 sub- jects who were given 14 hours of group counseling over a seven week period. Estimates of changes in subjects' "men- tal health" were made from an omnibus battery which included the following: a Picture Story Test, Brownfains's Self- Rating Inventory, a modified Behavior Rating Scale, and the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (EPPS). Results at the post test interval indicated no change on the Picture Story Test, no change on the Self-Rating Inventory, less desirable scores on the Behavioral Rating Scale, and changes on four of 15 scales of the EPPS. At the six month follow- up interval no control group comparisons were available and results indicated non-significant gains on the Self-Rating Inventory, non-significant gains on the Behavior Rating Scale, and changes on three of the 15 scales of the EPPS. The Picture Story Test was not administered at this time. Fourteen months after the conclusion of the treatment, 27 of the subjects completed a questionnaire or were inter- viewed, and 14 subjects were available to take the Picture Story Test. Results on the latter showed significant increase in self-acceptance, though no such gain in accep- tance of others. Of the 27 subjects who completed the questionnaire, 22 reported that group counseling had been beneficial, two said it had not, and three reported that it had, in fact, hurt them. The authors conclude that, while strict interpretation of the hypotheses tested would prove negative, on the basis of the combined data, the evi- dence may be sufficient to prove the value of group coun- seling. The evidence, as reported above, clearly does not support this conclusion in any compelling fashion. Hurst and Jensen (1968) reported on six subjects who were given the Human Development Institute's Relation- ship Improvement Program along with ten weekly T-Group ses- sions. These subjects were compared with others who par- ticipated in a reading/discussion seminar or who received no treatment. Assessment devices included the Berger Scales of Acceptance of Self and of Others, the Attitude Toward Self (As) and Attitude Toward Others (Ac) scales of the MMPI, and Self-Acceptance and Other-Acceptance scales adapted from the Butler-Haigh Q-sort. In addition, pre and post-test measures were taken on counselees seen by the subjects to determine if any differential gains in aca- demic achievement or personal and social adjustment were evident. Results indicated that the treatment group of interest made increases significantly higher on scores of the As and A0 scales of the MMPI. No other significant findings were reported. Significant gains were reported (McKinnon, 1969) on one of five scales of a Thematic Apperception Instru- ment and one of five dimensions on a Counselor Response Scale by students who participated in group counseling while enrolled in practicum (N = 7). No such gains were found for counselor trainees in group counseling who were not in practicum, nor for those in practicum but not in group counseling. More important, no significant differ- ences between treatments and control were found on any of the measures administered at post-test. Wirt, Betz, and Engle (1969) investigated the ef- fects of group counseling on the self concepts of counselor trainees. Seventeen subjects received 14 hours of group counseling over a fourteen week period. Statistical analy- ses revealed a positive change in the measured self con- cepts of the experimental group. However, differences be- tween the experimental and control groups after treatment remained non-significant. An interpretation of the results is difficult in this case because scores of subjects who dropped out of the control group are included in the pre- test data and other data suggest a lack of initial equiva- lence between groups. An attempt to compare changes in inter- and intra- personal functioning as the result of participation in T- Group activities and participation in a didactically ori- ented class was reported by Eiben (1971). An unspecified number of beginning counselor candidates were given group counseling or traditional classroom instruction on group methods for two hours per week for approximately 14 weeks. Assessment of level of functioning was done using the Per- sonal Orientation Inventory (POI). Comparison of mean scores at pre- and post-testing revealed significant gains on 11 of the 12 scales of the POI for the group participa- ting in counseling. On the same basis, the group receiv- ing didactic instruction showed significant gains on four scales. When gains between pre- and post-testing on each scale were compared between each group, four favored the didactic group and eight favored the counseled group--one of which reached significance. Inspection of the data in- dicates that this finding is the result of an unexplained, isolated decrease on this scale by the didactic group, rather than the result of positive gain by those in group counseling. Eiben's interpretation, that the concept being measured cannot be taught but must be lived, seems rather strained. No differences were apparent at post- testing between the two groups on any of the 12 scales. Other less ambitious studies have been reported. Seegars and McDonald (1963) provided an "interaction group" experience to eight student counselors in an at- tempt to increase self knowledge and perceptive skills. While the gross data were in support of the expressed pur- poses, subjective evaluation of questionnaire responses 10 form the basis of their evidence. Role change during group counseling was studied by Gazda and Bonney (1965) using counselor trainees as subjects. After 20 hours of treat- ment during a seven week period, a significant increase was detected in the roles of information giver, interpreter, non-participant, and aggressor. Methodological problems restricted the implications which the authors were able to generate. Reddy (1970) tested to see if six students en- tering practicum would perceive each other as having greater levels of empathy, regard, and congruence as a result of four days of intensive group counseling. Further, would clients of these group members perceive these levels the same as other group members did? Considering the small num- ber of subjects involved, the lack of controls, and the sta- tistical procedures employed, little confidence can be placed on the findings suggested. Several studies have pointed up issues which may have special relevance for the topic of this study. Per- sonal report of benefits gained as a result of group coun- seling must be looked upon with caution. Prediger and Baumann (1970) reported a study in which 30 groups of stu- dents were given group counseling one 40 minute period per week for approximately nine months. Although no gains were found on any of the 30 outcome variables included in the de- sign, subjects receiving the treatment condition consistently reported the experience as having greater benefit than did 11 subjects in the controls. This sort of problem is illus- trated by the tortuous discussion in the Leib and Snyder (1967) study in which students who were removed from a required class for special treatment showed similar pos- itive gains in psychological adjustment whether given group counseling or an academic lecture. The latency phenomenon ascribed to the Gazda and Ohlsen (1961) follow up data, it is suggested, may be a less parsimonious ex- planation of just this effect. Another issue to be considered concerns the use of indirect measures of inferred psychological constructs. Inspection of the studies reviewed above shows the prepon- derance of self-report measures found in the literature. Two questions are pertinent. Is group counseling assoc- iated with changes in self-reports of counselors in train- ing? Are changes in self-reports associated with predicted behavioral correlates? Apostal and Muro (1970) gave 20 prospective counselors approximately 50 hours of group counseling in 13 weeks with the purpose of increasing self understanding. Experimental and control subjects were pre- and post-tested on the EPPS and the Motivation Analysis Test (MAT). In addition, personality sketches were devel- oped for each subject on the basis of the pre-test data. Findings indicated that subjects experiencing group coun- seling changed significantly more than control subjects on two of the 25 scales involved, results which could be 12 attained by chance. In addition, self understanding, oper- ationalized as self recognition ability, was not changed when measured by the ability to correctly identify one's own personality sketch. The same sort of problem recurs in a study reported by Culbert, Clark, and Bobele (1968). Two groups of uni- versity students were given 28 hours of group counseling over a 14 week period with the purpose of promoting authen— tic interaction and increased self-awareness. Assessment procedures involved the use of the POI and the Problem Ex- pression Scale (PES), a measure of self-aware behavior. Significant changes toward improved psychological adjust- ment were found for one group, while the other group was found to be in an advanced state of adjustment on the pre- test and post-test. However, POI changes for individuals, regardless of group, did not correlate with indexes for measuring individuals' change on the PES. This would in- dicate a discrepancy between the way in which subjects re- port their self—percepts and report their behavior. This raises a serious question about the practice of using in- direct measures to infer therapeutic growth or gain. According to Bonney (1965), it would be poor judg- ment to ignore pressures toward conformity and to regard gains or growth through group counseling solely as the re- sult of intrapsychic phenomena. Drawing from research in social psychology, he points out that the elements which 13 cause unusual susceptibility to conformity pressures, emo- tionality and novelty, are both present in the group coun- seling setting. In these situations an individual becomes highly dependent on group norms or authority statements for his response choice. One possible explanation for self- reports changing while behavior does not may be that the individual has been persuaded to describe himself in altered ways without the necessity for changing habitual patterns of acting. The limited support which the studies cited supply to the notion that personality change of significance does indeed occur in group counseling with prospective counselors may be further questioned when the special characteristics of these subjects are considered. Counselors in training may be more sophisticated toward the nuances and implica- tions of what transpires in the group than most participants. They may also recognize that, besides being a potention "growth" experience, the group may represent an evaluative procedure in which they are, in effect, on display. Fur- thermore, they may observe certain attitudes, values, state- ments, and styles of behavior reacted to positively, while others are punished or ignored. Kramer (1968) has reported a positive conditioning effect on three verbal response classes when reinforced concurrently by the leader in a small group setting. The relative sophistication of coun- selors would increase the likelihood that even subtle cues 14 could be discriminated. This would seem to point up the possibility that leaders of groups made up of counselor trainees may well be involved in shaping the verbal behav- ior of these individuals toward the acceptance of key at- titude and value statements consistent with the model of psychological adjustment held by that leader, and on which the various measurement devices may be based. In short, influences other than those associated with simple group counseling may be operating to the ex- tent that they constitute valid alternative explanations for the meager results currently available. Such explan- ations could provide hard data concerning the utility of group counseling for counselor education programs. Hypotheses In view of the above, the following questions, as they apply to counselor trainees, were investigated. Are there different effects as a result of participation in group counseling or participation in lecture-discussions when the model of psychological adjustment used in the measuring instrument is: (1) consistent with the theo- retical orientation of the group leader; (2) not consis- tent with the theoretical orientation of the group leader; or (3) explicitly taught in a lecture-discussion class. Further, are any effects shown to generalize to simulated counseling behavior on the kind of performance measure employed? 15 To investigate these items of interest the follow- ing hypotheses were tested: I. Subjects who receive any of the proposed treatments will receive higher scores on the self-actualization test than will control subjects. II. There will be no difference in the self-actualization scores of subjects between any of the proposed treat- ments. III. There will be no difference between subjects who receive any of the proposed treatments and control subjects on the discrimination test. Overview The organization of the study is as follows. In Chapter II, the experimental subjects, treatments, and in- struments are described, along with a statement of the spe- cific procedures which were employed. Testable hypotheses are developed and the research design is identified. In addition, the type of statistical treatment and the level of significance to be used are described. In Chapter III the results of the data analysis are reported. Chapter IV includes a discussion of these results and a summary of the study. CHAPTER II EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY After consideration of the issues and hypotheses presented in the previous Chapter, organization of an experimental study was carried out. Subjects Students enrolled in the master's degree level counseling practicum at Michigan State University during the spring term of 1971 were the treatment subjects for this study. Of the 69 students initially available, 63 students became subjects of the study. Six students were excluded for the following reasons: unable to take the pre-test or post-test (2 gs), reluctance to take part in the study treatment sessions (2 Ss), failure to meet the required attendance criteria for the treatment sessions (1 S), and termination of enrollment status (1 S), In addition, nine students were identified as being members of the same counselor training program, but were not en- rolled in practicum. These subjects constituted an inac- tive control group. 16 17 The resulting group of experimental subjects con- sisted of 37 trainees in the school counselor program and 26 trainees in the rehabilitation counselor program. In- cluded were 29 males and 34 females with ages ranging from 21 to 48 years, a median age of 24 years, and a mean age of 25.6 years. Twenty-nine subjects had received bachelor's de- grees from Michigan State University, 11 received their degree from other colleges or universities in Michigan, and 23 from colleges or universities outside the state. Psychology was the most common undergraduate major (15 gs). Sixteen subjects were prepared in secondary education, with a variety of major fields represented. Other areas strongly represented were social sciences (12 gs) and elementary edu- cation (8 Ss). Less common majors were art (2 Se), foreign language (2 gs), and business (2 Se). Thirty-four subjects reported no teaching experience, fourteen had one year or less, and three had more than four years teaching experience. Treatments Five treatment conditions were employed. Treatment 1 Group counseling involving affective interaction was used to promote the personal development of group mem- bers. The leader was guided by the definition of Gazda, 18 Duncan, and Meadows (1967), which emphasizes interpersonal communication focused on conscious thought and behavior. In this model personal concerns are shared among individ- uals who are not debilitated by their problems. Attention is directed toward feelings rather than cognitive data. Permissiveness, orientation to reality, and affective dis- closure are involved in a setting of mutual trust, under- standing, and support. The theoretical orientation of the group leader was consistent with the construct of self- actualization as a model of psychological adjustment. The remarks of the group leader could be associated with a point of view in which an ideal model of psychological well-being is valued. Treatment 2 Group counseling involving affective interaction was used to promote the personal development of group mem— bers. The leader was guided by the definition of Gazda, Duncan, and Meadows (1967), which emphasizes interpersonal communication focused on conscious thought and behavior. In this model personal concerns are shared among individ— uals who are not debilitated by their problems. Attention is directed toward feelings rather than cognitive data. Permissiveness, orientation to reality, and affective dis- closure are involved in a setting of mutual trust, under- standing, and support. The theoretical orientation of the 19 group leader was not consistant with the construct of self- actualization as a model of psychological adjustment. The remarks of the group leader could be associated with a point of view in which social-learning theory is valued. Treatment 3 A seminar in lecture-discussion format was used to present content which emphasized the importance of ideal models of psychological adjustment and their relationship to positive counselor behavior. The time was divided about equally between leader presentations and group discussions. Printed material Was handed out from Carkhuff and Berenson (1967), Bugental (1965), and Maslow (1967). Films were shown of Perls and Rogers interviewing a client and ex- plaining their respective approaches. Concepts such as "fully-functioning," "authentic," "whole," and "self- actualized" were discussed. Research and opinion which related personality constructs such as these to favorable counselor performance was presented. The leader assumed a directive role, avoided focusing attention on affective comments, and emphasized the cognitive content involved. Treatment 4 A seminar in which varied activities were presented served as the active control treatment. Two criteria were used to eliminate inappropriate activities for this 20 treatment purpose: it should not require predominately affective interaction; it should not focus on any model of ideal psychological adjustment or related terminology. Representative activities which were presented include the following learning experiences: discussion of prob- lems encountered in various practicum field settings; training in providing objective, helpful feedback for the improvement of counseling strategies; practice in using this skill in response to films, video tapes, and audio tapes presented by the leader and seminar members; instruction in specific techniques such as problem solv- ing strategies, relaxation training, assertive training techniques, and behavior contracting; discussion of mem- bers' on-going counseling cases. The leader acted as an instructor and resource person. Treatment 5 The inactive control group was made up of students from the same training program who were currently enrolled in some other university course but were not enrolled in practicum. No contact was made with these subjects other than for pretesting and post-testing. Group counseling under Treatment 1 employed two separate leaders (one for each sub-group), both of whom were advanced doctoral students with considerable exper- ience in group processes. The training of one was in 21 clinical psychology and his expressed theoretical orienta- tion was Gestalt. The training of the other was in coun- seling psychology and his statement of theoretical persua- sion indicated a commitment to the views of Perls, primar- ily and Rogers, secondarily, in terms of group procedures. The experience of the former included 110 hours as a group participant, 140 hours supervised training in group leader- ship, and 190 hours leadership of therapeutic and personal growth groups. The later reported some 400 hours as a group member, 96 hours supervised training in group leader- ship, and 160 hours leadership of group counseling sessions. Group counseling under Treatment 2 was carried out by a leader with a doctorate in counseling psychology. His stated theoretical orientation was behavioral--based on Skinner, Bandura, and Ulmann and Krasner. The experience of this leader included 90 hours as a group participant, 75 hours of supervised leadership training at the post- doctoral level, and 900 hours of leadership of group coun- seling sessions. To illustrate the differences in leader responses between treatment groups 1 and 2, the following excerpts have been selected from tape recordings which were made of the third, fifth, and seventh group sessions. The first three excerpts are from Treatment 1. l. M But I'm still here. M: humm. . . you know you could call this bullshit if you want to, but I really 22 do . . . ahh . . . I really do worry about what the other person's going to feel. That's in it, too . . . I know . . . even if I am going to check someone off my list, I won't, you know . . . maybe there's some- thing about me in there, too . . . you know . . . I am sure, but I really do care about, you know, I don't want . . . I am sure that that feels different for you to say that than it did the first time you said about not wanting to see someone crumr ble. Didn't that, the time you said it now, didn't it feel different to you? Yeh. Ok. So you know I believe you. Hmm. It's too bad that you had to be hammered that hard . . . to give up those real car- ing feelings that you do have for people. Somehow I feel as though I've . . . there's an irreverant side to me . . . somehow I feel as though I've just been saved. I expect you to call those feelings in you all kinds of dirty names. I really expect you to do that. You've been running from it for too damned long to face them flush right now. You can call them anything you want, I guess, but I . . . it won't shake my trust any. . . parts of you. That you kind of gave up when you did that change. uh huh. Umm, do you know what those are? . . . I think they are the things that glow out of her, too. Might be. 'Cause . . . ahh, you know, appreciation for music sounds to me like kind of one of those things . . . Yea, I had that pulled out of me, too. Except I realized the guy that did that to me, and I've never really forgiven him for that. 'Caust that was my one way that I . . . you know, when we talk about that self-actualization thing, that was the one thing that I ever did that I really self- actualized on. 'Cause I was an all-state band type thing. And, ahh, like, you get all of those instruments around you, you 23 know . . . and it's like a feeling . . . like it's wierd . . . you have to do it . . . and somehow I had that pulled out of me, too. You meant when you were playing it was really an emotional thing for you. Yea. Right. And you felt good at it. You were good at it. You know, I either trust them, or I don't, you know. And I do shut people off. If I find out that someone has done some- thing to me, that's it. It is a self pre- servation thing. How in the hell do you know? Like . . . how can you make sense out of that situa- tion, and know what her intent was and know how much the other chick distorted it to you. You can't ever know that. And unless you go back to her with the feelings you've got about her, and get that straight . . . there's nothing you can be clear about. The only thing you can be clear about is what is between the two of you. 0k, it's not right, but it works. Oh, shit. It works to what end? It keeps . . . like everybody who is around me I can trust because of the pro- cess. If I can't trust them, they're not around me. Maybe it's your . . . Do you completely ignore her? Since then . . . I want you to see what you're saying is crap, because you haven't ignored her. No, no, no. It's in here. That's where it happens. Ok, well . . . And it's not at all fair, you're right. The sad part is that it is not fair to you. I mean, it's not going to kill her. But it is not fair to you. I mean, because you got scared once, you know, you never risk again with her of having something. You shut the door to her forever. Hmm. And so you get cheated out of whatever you could have with her. 24 The following three excerpts are from Treatment 2: l. L: Well, I like it. Ah . . . my wife . . . she likes it. My daughter likes it. I get a lot of reinforcement there. And uhh . . . if I don't . . . ahh . . . I . . . I am still struggling with it as you can see, because to keep it looking what I think is nice you ought to wash it about every two to three days, which, I get busy doing other things and I don't like to wash it that often. When my wife rein- forces me, she tells me, "It really doesn't take that much time. And you look so nice after you've done it . . . keep your hair washed." It's not dirty . . . it just gets . . . oily. And I guess it is dirty, too, but not real dirty. Ah . . . this. . . as I was saying, is me, ah, was a contrived thing. I thought this out. How can I change myself to where I liked myself bet- ter, and part of it was clothes, uhh . . . physical appearance, uhh, but this changed me internally too . . . because I felt very good about myself, ahh . . . in terms of you know . . . ahh . . . I get enough stroking from other people about it, espe- cially from my family. That's enough really . . . so I changed myself. M: Yes, but you must have thought about it, you know, like before you went ahead and actually, it came consciously. There must have been something that was starting to bother you and grate and irritate under- neath. L: There are no little people inside of me, as I said, M: Oh. L: causing things to happen. I learn; I relearn; I unlearn. There are no devils; there are no angels. There are no little men. There's nothing inside of me. 2. M: . . . in the school system where they came in every morning with a clipboard and de- manded to know what I had done the week before trying to teach human relations. M: Who's they? M: The supervision, the principal and the administration. They bugged the heck out of me. Because I don't think you teach Mo L 25 human relations, anyway. I felt a great deal of pressure there . . . and I might add a great deal of hostility. So you are your prior learning history. You rigfiffhoW'feel a particular way toward Mary . . . which you can intellectualize . . but you can't really own your own feelings. You know she's too . . . to me too . . . Well, why are you giving us this history then for? I don't know. You trying to tell Mary something? I really don't know. Probably trying to explain to me why you are like you are. Don't you think he's trying to tell himself something? He's trying to put it all to- gether for himself . . . You know, I can't tie this in with being sensitive to people. I think you're delud- ing yourself. Something different everyday? Ok, but I think so far I'm not particularly fond of . . . It's all right. I'd like to find out if there is something that I would like. Does that matter? Do you have to be fond of us? Do I have to be. Yah. Before I make the judgment that he's a no good guy I'd like to find out if there is something there. Keep watching each week, maybe you'll find something. (laughter) Maybe you can shape him . . . find the way you like and reward him somehow. Maybe I like him. What's your word for it? Although different leader viewpoints between Treat- ments 1 and 2 were assumed, no systematic assessment of such differences was attempted. However, it was of critical im- portance that affective interaction be similar in all groups involved in group counseling, both under Treatment 1 and 26 Treatment 2. An experimental check adapted from Betz (1963), was employed for this purpose. Tape recordings of one and one-half hour duration were made from the third, fifth, and seventh group sessions. Six units of analysis were randomly selected from each session for each group. The individual unit selected for analysis was a group leader's response along with the member(s) response which immediately preceded and precipitated the leader's remark (Appendix A). This procedure produced 72 member-leader response units, 36 from Treatment 1 and an equal number from Treat- ment 2, which were randomly assigned a location number (1 to 72) and converted to typescripts. The typescripts were submitted to two judges (advanced Ph.D. candidates) with instructions to identify the leader response as likely to promote affective or cognitive interaction (Appendix B). Ratings of the judges were in agreement on 61 of the 72 items (85%). Comparison of evaluations assigned to each treatment group was done for combined judge's ratings by using a chi-square test with a two-way contingency table (Hays, 1963). A significant chi-square was interpreted as evidence that leader responses differed on the affective— cognitive dimension between treatment groups 1 and 2. Table 2.1 Test for Difference in Leader Responses Observed Necessary df p Chi-square Chi-square 3.836 3.842 1 N.S. 27 As shown in Table 2.1, no significant differences were found. It was concluded from these data that the level of affective response provided by the leader under either treatment condition was not different. Procedures All students enrolled in the counseling practicum were randomly assigned to one of the active treatment groups. Thus, the class was divided into four activities, correspond- ing to the treatments, which were carried on simultaneously throughout the term. Since there was a morning and an after- noon section of this class, each treatment consisted of two sub-groups, ranging from seven to ten members. Pre-testing on the POI and the Discrimination test was done during the first class meeting. During the second and ninth class meeting treatment activities of one hour duration were held. During each of the six intervening weekly classes, treatments of two hours duration took place. Therefore, each group was treated for fourteen hours over an.eight week period. Post-testing on the POI and Discrim- ination test was done immediately after the eighth treat- ment session. Instruments Assessment of subjects' level of psychological well- being was done using the Personal Orientation Inventory 28 (POI), an instrument developed by Shostrom (1966) to mea- sure the self—actualization level of subjects. It is a 150 item questionnaire of forced dichotomous choices in- volving behavior and value judgments. Scores are reported on two major scales involving time orientation (Tc) and other/inner motivation (I). The sum of Tc + I represents the total self-actualization level. Ten sub-scales, each of which is considered to be an important conceptual element of self-actualization, are also provided. 1. Self-Actualizing Values (SAV): affirmation of values held by self-actualized persons. 2. Existentiality (Ex): reported ability to react situationally without rigid adherence to prin- ciples. 3. Feeling Reactivity_(Fr): reported sensitivity to responsiveness to one's own feelings. 4. Spontaneity (S): reported preference to re- act spontaneously rather than cautiously. 5. Self-Regard (Sr): affirmation of self because of one's worth or strength. 6. Self-Acceptance (Sa): affirmation of self in spite of weakness or deficiencies. 7. View of the Nature of Man (Nc): reported con- structive view of the nature of man. 29 8. Synergy (Sy): reported perspective which avoids dichotomies. 9. Acceptance of Aggression (A): reported pre- ference for owning one's aggressiveness rather than a denial or repression of one's aggressiveness. 10. Capacity for Intimate Contact (C): reported preference for developing intimate relationships with others which are characterized by a lack of mutual expectations or feelings of obligation. Validity data for the POI has usually been of two sorts. In one kind, groups of subjects are identified as being well adjusted or poorly adjusted on a basis such as clinical judgment (Shostrom, 1964), process in therapy (Shostrom and Knapp, 1966), or the fact of hospitalization. Results indicate that mean scores of such groups differ in the hypothesized directions from the comparison groups that were used. Another kind of study has attempted to estab- lish the concurrent validity of the POI through the rela- tionship with other personality tests. Results indicate that relationships exist between POI measures and the Social I.E. (Si), Depression (D), and Psychastenia (Pt) scales of the MMPI (Shostrom and Knapp, 1966) and between POI measures and the construct of neuroticism employed in the Eysenck Personality Inventory (Knapp, 1965). Test-retest reliability coefficients of .71 and .84 for the To and I scales, respectively, have been 30 reported (Klavetter and Mogar, 1967). Reliability of the POI with the subjects of this study, as estimated by the Kuder-Richardson (KR-20) formula, was .89 and .90 for pre- test and post-test administrations, respectively. Of secondary interest, the ability to correctly identify the potential effectiveness of various counselor responses was assessed using the Carkhuff (1969) Discrim- ination Test (D/T). This device consists of sixteen client stimulus items, each of which is followed by four hypothet- ical counselor responses. The subject rates each counselor response on a nine point scale in terms of potential effec- tiveness. A score is derived on the basis of the summed absolute deviation from the correct rating, a value deter- mined by expert.concensus. For purposes of this study, the task presented by this instrument, rapid evaluation of a counselor response, may be regarded as a simulation of the decision process encountered during actual counseling when the counselor is formulating a response to a previous client remark. Carkhuff (1969) has suggested that the ability to discriminate responses of potentially greater effectiveness from those of lesser facilitative utility is a necessary, though not sufficient, element in the ability to commun- icate effectively with clients. Studies of discriminative ability (Carkhuff, Collingwood, and Renz, 1969; Carkhuff, Friel, and Kratochvil, 1969) indicate that subjects rated 31 high in facilitative communication are high in discrimin- ative ability, although discriminative level is not directly related to communicative ability. Consistent with this thinking and the concept of the "whole" person (Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967), improved intra-personal functioning would be reflected in increased discriminative skill. In addition, subjects were asked to evaluate the activity (treatment) in which they took part. An evaluation form (Appendix C) was provided as a means to systematize the responses on the dimensions of value, enjoyment, and as a specific learning experience. Finally, subjects receiving group counseling were asked to keep a log of their reactions to each session. This was done to generate data for non-controlled post-hoc analysis. Testable Hypotheses In order to determine the differential effect of the treatments which were used on the measures employed, it was necessary to demonstrate, first, that differences existed after treatment between experimental and control groups. Should such a difference appear, it would require that differences between experimental treatments be inves- tigated. To examine these relationships the following hypotheses were formulated: 32 There is no difference in post-test scores between subjects in groups receiving experimental and control treatments. If not, then 2. T1 - ______ = 0 There is no difference in post-test scores between subjects in groups receiving Treatment 1 those receiv- ing Treatment 2 and 3. 3. T1 - T = 0 There is no difference in post-test scores between subjects in groups receiving Treatment 1 and Treatment 2. Since inactive control subjects (T5) were not admin- istered the D/T, hypothesis 1 on this measure was formulated in the alternative form given above. Design and Statistical Analysis The study was intended to compare differences between groups after, and as a result of, participation in one of three experimental treatments. A 5 x 2 matrix, with groups nested within treatments, was utilized. The design, a 33 variant of the pre-test, post-test, control group pattern described in Campbell and Stanley (1966), may be graphically represented as shown in Table 2.2 Table 2.2 Graphic Representation of the Research Design at R 01 T1 06 Key: R 02 T2 07 0l . . . 05= Pre-test R 03 T3 08 T1 . . . T3 = Experimental treatment R 04 T4 09 T4 = Placebo treatment 05 (T5) 010 (T5) = Inactive control 06 . . . 010 = Post-test The initial statistical treatment was a multivar- iate analysis of covariance using planned comparisons in which pre-test scores were used as covariates. Post-test scores and all POI scales were the dependent variables. Analysis was across treatments, with groups nested. Following this, the most pertinent measures, Tc, I, and D/T score, were subjected to overall univariate ANCOVA analysis for main effects due to treatment. In the former case the alpha level was .05. However, since separate tests were used in the univariate analysis, the alpha level was set at .017 in order to maintain the in- tegrity of the test. 34 Summary Counselor trainees were assigned to one of five treatment conditions: 1. group counseling in which the leader's model of psychological adjustment was consistent with the measurement device 2. group counseling in which the leader's model was not consistent with the assumptions of the mea- surement device 3. a seminar in which the model of adjustment implicit in the measurement device was given a favor- able presentation 4. a placebo treatment 5. an inactive control group. Pre- and post-testing on the Personal Orientation Inven- tory and the Discrimination Test was carried out. A 5 x 2 design matrix was adopted and provision made to test the dependent variables by both multivariate and univariate analysis of covariance. The basic hypoth- esis, that experimental subjects would receive higher scores after treatment than control subjects, was presented. CHAPTER III ANALYSIS OF RESULTS Statistical analyses were calculated at the Michi- gan State University Computer Center on the Control Data 3600 computer system. Data reported below were generated by a multivariate analysis of covariance program developed by Finn (1967). Preliminary Data Mean scores at pre- and post-testing on the two major scales of the Personal Orientation Inventory (POI) and on the Discrimination Test (D/T) for all treatment groups are shown in Table 3.1 Complete POI sub-scale scores are reported in Appendix D. Mean, variance, and standard deviation of all measures for all subjects are listed in Appendix E. An initial test of the relationship between the dependent variable and the specified covariates was per- formed in order to determine the appropriateness of the covariance model. For the POI, multiple regression coef- ficients ranged from .60 to .86 for predicting each variable from the set of covariables. The overall chi-square test of the hypothesis of no association between the dependent 35 36 Table 3.1 Summary of Pre- and Post-test Scores of Major Interest. T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 Pre-test Tc 17.81 16.13 16.06 17.73 18.78 I 88.81 89.88 88.75 89.40 89.00 D/T 53.50 49.12 55.37 45.10 -- Post-test Tc 17.63 17.88 17.63 18.20 18.22 I 94.50 98.69 95.86 94.60 93.67 D/T* 45.00 44.90 44.40 40.67 -- Gain Tc - .18 1.75 1.57 .47 - .56 I 5.69 8.81 7.11 5.20 4.67 D/T 8.50 4.32 10.97 4.43 -- *Note: Decreased D/T scores indicate gain. variables and covariates produced a value of 351.2 which, with 144 degrees of freedom, is significant beyond .0001. For the D/T the multiple regression coefficient was .59 for predicting post-test score from pre-test score. Hence, in both cases, it was concluded that ANCOVA was appropriate. Treatment Effects Hypothesis 1: There is no difference in post-test scores between subjects in groups receiving experimental and control treatments. 37 Results of the multivariate analysis of hypothesis 1 using POI scores are shown in Table 3.2. Since a signifi- cant F-ratio was not obtained, the hypothesis of no differ- ence remains in effect. This is interpreted as indicating that no difference exists after treatment between experimen- tal and control subjects as measured by the set of POI scales taken as a whole. Table 3.2 Multivariate Test of Hypothesis 1: POI Scores F-ratio df p .484 12,39 <.91 Given this result, it was decided that the scales of secondary importance be eliminated from further inves- tigation and that the basic scales of the POI, Tc and I, be subjected to an overall ANCOVA analysis to determine whether any treatment effects were present in the data. As shown in Table 3.3, results were negative. This would indicate that no difference exists after treatment between any combination of treatment groups, as measured by the major scales of the POI. Results of the overall ANCOVA analysis of the D/T data are shown in Table 3.4. No significant effects were found. This would indicate that no difference exists 38 after treatment between any combination of treatment groups, as measured by the Discrimination test. Table 3.3 Overall ANCOVA Analysis of Major POI Scales MS MS Scale Between Within F p Tc 3.19 4.63 .688 <.604 I 115.28 46.01 2.506 <.0515 df = 4, 60 Table 3.4 Overall ANCOVA Analysis of Discrimination Test MS MS Between Within F p 51.76 129.34 .40 <.753 df = 3, 54 In total, these data confirm that no treatment effects could be demonstrated in this study using the mea- sures which were employed. Therefore, hypotheses 2 and 3 are answered in the negative by default and require no further data. 39 Non-experimental Data It should be clear that further data do not speak to the empirical questions which were investigated. They may, however, provide some basis for improving future in- vestigations. Pre- and Post-test Comparison Casual observation appeared to indicate an increase in POI scores over all subjects between pre- and post-test- ing. In addition, a similar decline in D/T scores was noted. Whether this difference represented chance variation was tested by ANOVA for D/T and combined I and Tc scores. As indicated in Table 3.5, a significant increase had occurred in both cases. Table 3.5 Test for Pre- and Post-test Differences. ms MS Between Within F p D/T 3087.00 161.54 19.11 <.0001 df = 1, 62 901 3741.13 70.79 52.35 <.0001 df = 1, 71 Group Counseling Logs Participants in group counseling were asked to keep a log of their reactions to group sessions. Although 40 they were assured anonymity and were asked not to sign their name, they were also informed that handing in this material was voluntary. Unfortunately, two of the four groups turned in such few returns that comparison of reactions was ruled out. Subjective appraisal of the limited responses seemed to confirm a sequence of stages or phases in group develop- ment similar to those described by Foley and Bonney (1965), and Tuckman (1965). Also, the effect of a recognized set number of sessions on member reactions, as noted by Mahler (1971), seemed present. Thus, while the intention of pre- senting logs as voluntary was to avoid an atmosphere of coercion, the result was to eliminate the log as a source of useful information. Subject Evaluation Three weeks following the termination of treat- ments, subjects were contacted in another setting and asked to evaluate the activity in which they took part. Forty- nine of the 63 subjects (78%) were located and responded to a standardized questionnaire (Appendix C). Thirty-five in- dicated that their activity (treatment) was a valuable learning experience; eleven did not. Thirty-six felt that it was enjoyable; eleven said no. To a global evaluation of the benefit which they attributed to the activity, 21 indicated it to have had much worth, 20 stated it was some- what worthwhile, and eight felt it to be of little value. 41 Marked differences appeared when these results were analyzed on a group basis. Group 1 under Treatment 1 and group 7 under Treatment 4 had decidely negative evaluations. For instance, they accounted for five of the eight opinions that their activity was of little worth and eight of the eleven reactions that it was not a valuable learning experience. The remaining six groups expressed strongly positive opinions. Summary Evidence indicating a correlation between depen- dent variables and specified covariates was found, thus supporting the intention to use ANCOVA as the statistical model. No evidence of difference after treatment between experimental and control subjects was found using the POI scales in a multivariate analysis of covariance. Further, no difference between any treatment groups was evident using the two major scales of the POI in an overall ANCOVA test. In addition, no difference between any treatment groups was found at post-test on the D/T using an overall ANCOVA pro- cedure. Concluding that no treatment effect could be demon- strated, hypotheses 2 and 3 were not tested. Non-experimental data was also reported. One source, group counseling logs, appeared to lose any utility due to the small number which were returned. However, gains over 42 all subjects between pre- and post-testing were noted on both total POI score and D/T score. Subject evaluation of participation in the various treatment conditions was gen- erally positive, with apparent differences between groups. CHAPTER IV SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION Summary The purpose of this study was to assess the ef- fect which short-term group counseling and a cognitive learning experience may have on a measure of self-actual- ization and, secondarily, on a simulation of counselor per- formance. Much of the existing literature in counselor ed- ucation seems to accept the notion that group counseling has utility in counselor education programs because of the affective learning or reorientation which may take place, rather than development of more specific competencies. Changes on various personality measures, of which the Per- sonal Orientation Inventory (POI) is representative, are considered sufficient evidence to support this position. It was suggested that influences other than those usually attributed to group counseling may account in part for changes found on the measures which have been used. If so, the basis for determining the utility of group counsel- ing in counselor education programs would need to be re- examined. 43 44 Subjects of the study were 72 master's degree candidates in counseling at Michigan State University. Three experimental conditions were developed. In one, subjects participated in group counseling in which the leader's views were consistent with the model of adjust- ment on which the POI is based. In another, subjects participated in group counseling in which the leader's views were inconsistent with this model. In the third, subjects participated in a lecture-discussion seminar in which the model of adjustment on which the POI is based was presented in a favorable manner. Placebo treatment and inactive control conditions were included. Treatments were maintained for 14 hours over eight sessions. The ob- jective was to compare the effects which would result from a cognitive presentation and from experience in group coun- seling with and without the opportunity for extraneous learning which would tend to improve scores on the measure which was employed. A 5 x 2 design matrix was developed, with groups nested within treatments. Pre-test scores were used as covariates and provision was made for both multivariate and univariate analyses. The basic hypothesis was that experimental subjects would receive higher scores after treatment than would control subjects. Secondary hypothe- ses were designed to determine the effect of each experimen- tal treatment. 45 Preliminary analysis revealed the ANCOVA model to be appropriate. However, no differences were apparent at the .05 level between experimental and control subjects with POI scores used in a multivariate test. Further in- vestigation using the major scales of the POI and the Dis- crimination Test showed that no significant differences existed between any of the treatment groups on these mea- sures. Hence, it was concluded that no effects due to treatment could be demonstrated. Discussion The present study showed no significant treatment effects. Although some factors may remain hidden and in- teractions between known elements may remain undiscovered, recognized sources of potential error can be usefully re- viewed in retrospect. The results obtained may be attri- buted, at least, to the following possible contributory factors: theory, sampling, design and statistics, instru- mentation, and treatment. Each will be considered below. Theory A possible cause of the outcome of the study might be an error in, or misinterpretation of, the literature. In regard to interpretation, it seems clear that the literature, for the most part, takes for granted that personality may be affected by group counseling. Whether persons can be 46 measurably affected by group counseling, is difficult to assess. However, research reported in Chapter I suggests that some success with personality devices such as the POI has been demonstrated with counselor trainees. Since suf- ficient evidence is not available, this source of error must remain a possibility. Sample Sampling problems tend to be more severe in survey studies than in experimental research. However, the per- tinent question here concerns whether the subjects of this study differ systematically from those in earlier reported studies in any characteristic which might affect their per- formance on the measures which were employed. Unfortunately no direct comparison can be made. It is known that Michigan State University had no special selection criteria in effect when this class of candidates for the master's degree in counseling was admit- ted. Inspection of the data in Chapter II reveals no ap- parent discrepancy on the demographic dimensions reported which would indicate these subjects to be unrepresentative of counselor candidates at other universities. In sum, there is no evidence which would appear to indicate the presence of characteristics which would make these subjects a unique group. 47 Design and Statistical Treatment The general concern in this respect is whether any effects could have been distorted or obscured because of the way the study was organized or analyzed. There are at least two considerations relating to design and statistical matters which warrant comment. First, as indicated by the degrees of freedom which are reported, the individual subject has been treated as the experimental unit of analysis in this study. Although this cannot prOperly be justified, the procedure has become a con- vention in group counseling research at this stage of its development because doing so may allow weaker effects to be noticed. Therefore, to have done so in this study could have acted to cause spurious effects to appear, rather than acting to obscure real effects. Second, since Treatments 1 and 4 employed two lead- ers and Treatments 2 and 3 used only one, in the latter case treatment and leader are confounded. This is unfortunate because, particularly in Treatments 1 and 2, it is likely that the effectiveness with which individuals can deliver such a treatment may vary considerably. For instance, lead- er ineffectiveness could depress subject gains in some treat- ments, perhaps obscuring overall treatment effects. In this case, however, inspection of mean gain for Treatments 2 and 48 3 does not seem to support this suspicion. In fact, no group x treatment effect was discovered in any of the tests reported in Chapter III. It is concluded, then, that no factors related to the design or statistical procedures have been identified which would act to obscure real treatment effects. Instruments The discussion of problems associated with mea- surement will be limited to the POI since that instrument was the main concern of this study. The reliability of the POI with this group of subjects, as reported in Chapter II, seems adequate indeed. However, the use of a self report of this type with subjects sophisticated in respect to the concepts involved needs to be explored. For example, what might account for the general increase in scores which was observed? One possibility could be some change in the expectancy set of the subjects. Contrary to published results on the fakability of the POI (Foulds, 1971), preliminary results of a study in progress by this author indicate the POI to be highly susceptible to differential instructions given to beginning teacher candidates. If this, indeed, be the case, changing expec- tancy, desire to please, or other similar sets could sub- stantially alter post-test scores. A provocative consider- ation may be the fact that the two groups identified in 49 Chapter III as not valuing their treatment experience may be seen in Appendix D to have the least observed gain in total POI score. Other groups compared against these, in fact, would be significantly different. ISuch speculation is tempting. However, the ques- tion, often raised in the literature, of whether devices such as these are capable of measuring the kind and extent of change which might be expected from group counseling with counselor trainees remains unanswered. Treatments Under this rubric such concerns as the adequate presentation or control of the treatment conditions ought to be considered. A recurring issue, often invoked to explain a lack of anticipated findings, involves the necessary time for group counseling to have some measurable effect. The studies cited in Chapter I ranged from 50 to 14 hours of treatment administered over 14 weeks to four days. Zimpfer (1968) states that most group counseling research has been conducted with from 8 to 16 sessions. Gazda and Larsen's (1968) survey of group counseling research through 1967 found the average treatment to consist of 17 hours and 17 weeks. Reporting mean rather than median quantities is somewhat misleading because the existence of a few long- term studies acts to inflate the figures given. The 50 presentation of treatment in this study, 14 hours over 8 sessions, is probably of less duration than the typical study reported. Therefore, this must be considered a pos- sible factor contributing to the lack of treatment effects reported in Chapter III. Control of treatment conditions has been a serious and persistent problem in group counseling research. For purposes of this study a more precise description of group counseling was undesirable in terms of restricting the lead- ers to predetermined roles with prearranged activities. In- stead, the intent was to allow them flexibility within the boundaries which were provided. This was done in an attempt to provide a modal kind of treatment as a ground against which the other aspects of the experimental manipulation could be contrasted. On the other hand, while non-specific description of treatment does not preclude obtaining an effect, it acts to cast suspicion on the power of the treat- ment conditions if no effect is found. Contrary to what might be expected, the major prob- lem of controling treatment conditions may have occurred not within the counseled groups, Treatments 1 and 2, but within the placebo control, Treatment 4. As this study was orig- inally conceptualized, no problem was foreseen in controling the affective interchange level within Treatments 3 and 4. Therefore, no attempt was made to monitor this dimension as was provided for with Treatments 1 and 2. However, subjects 51 in group 2 under Treatment 4, initially reluctant partici- pants, were found on the Activity Evaluation Form to be particularly strong advocates of the personal qualities of their seminar leader. It seems possible then that this group became another, unidentified, experimental treatment. Interestingly, gains made by this group were noticeably higher than those of other groups designated as being con- trols. It therefore seems possible that problems asso- ciated with the presentation and control of treatments were encountered and may have contributed to the outcome of this study. Conclusion The essential question asked by this research con- cerned whether coqnitive learning might account for changes on personality measures used in group counseling studies with counselor trainees and, thereby, constitute a viable alternative explanation to the inference that psychological adjustment had taken place. Unfortunately, no answer is forthcoming since this study was unable to demonstrate dif- ferential levels of measured psychological well-being as a result of the treatments which were provided. The direct conclusion to be drawn on the basis of the results of this study is that participation in group counseling or the cognitive learning experience, as presented, 52 caused no additional increment on the measures which were used. Thus, in this case, treatment was no more effective than no treatment. While consideration must be given to the matters presented in the discussion above, it may also be true that group counseling is an ineffectual experience for purposes which depend upon personality assessment. Re- sults such as those reported here have brought the sugges- tion from Apostal and Muro (1970) "that group counseling as a practical procedure cannot be justified on the basis of important personality changes in prospective counselors. Perhaps, however, it may be justified on other grounds, such as contributing to effective training in group counsel- ing skills through actual participation in the process, (p. 62)." The issues presented point up the difficulty of research in counselor education as attempts are made to evaluate the kinds of experiences provided in training pro- grams. The basis for the utility of group counseling with counselor candidates remains unclear. LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES American Personnel and Guidance Association. Standards for the preparation of school Counselors. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1961, 32, 402-407. Anderson, D. G., & Cabianca, W. A. The "micro-lab": An introduction to group process. Counselor Educa- tion and Supervision, 1970, a, 299-300. Apostal, R. A., & Muro, J. J. Effects of group counsel- ing on self-reports and on self-recognition abil- ities of counselors in training. Counselor Edu- cation and Supervision, 1970, r0, 56-63. Association for Counselor Education and Supervision. Standards for the preparation of secondary school counselors--l967. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1967, 46, 96-106. - Betz, R. L. A study of the effects of two types of group counseling on the counseling performance of coun- selor candidates. Unpublished doctoral disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1963. Betz, R. L. Effects of group counseling as an adjunctive practicum experience. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969, r6, 528-533. Bonney, W. C. Pressures toward conformity in group coun- seling. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 43, 970-973. Bonney, W. C., & Gazda, G. M. Group counseling experi- ences: Reactions by counselor candidates. Coun- selor Education and Supervision, 1966, 5, 205:2II. Bugental, J. F. T. The search for authenticity. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1967. Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. Experimental and quasi- experimental designs for research. ChiCago: Rand McNally, 1966. 53 54 Carkhuff, R. Helping and human relations. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969. Carkhuff, R. R., & Berenson, B. G. Beyond counseling and therapy. New York: Holt, Rinéfiart & Winston, Carkhuff, R. R., Collingwood, T., & Renz, L. The effects of didactic training in discrimination upon trainee level of discrimination and communication. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1969, 8, 104-107. Carkhuff, R. R., Kratochvil, D., & Friel, T. The effects of professional training: The communication and discrimination of facilitative conditions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, r5, 68-74. Culbert, S. A., Clark, J. V., & Bobele, H. K. Measures of Eiben, change toward self-actualization in two sensitivity training groups. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, 13! 53-57. R. E. Impact of a participatory group experience on counselors in training. Paper given at American Personnel and Guidance Association, Atlantic City, April, 1971. ' Finn, J. Multivariance: Fortanyprogram for univariate and Foley, Foulds, Gazda, Gazda, Gazda, multivariate analysis of variance and covariance. State University of New York at Buffalo, 1967. W. J., & Bonney, W. C. A developmental model for counseling groups. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 44, 125-128. M. L., & Warhime, R. G. Effects of a "fake good" response set on a measure of self-actualization. Jgurnal of Counseling Psychology, 1971, r8, 279- 280. G. M., & Bonney, W. C. Effects of group counseling on role behavior of counselors in training. Coun- selor Education and Supervision, 1965, 4, l91-I97. G. M., Duncan, J. A., & Meadows, M. E. Group coun- seling and group procedures--report of a survey. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1967, 6, 305- 310. G. M., & Larsen, M. J. A comprehensive appraisal of group and multiple counseling research. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 1968, r, 57-132. 55 Gazda, G., & Ohlsen, M. The effects of short-term group counseling on prospective counselors. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1961, 42, 634-638. Hays, W. L. gratistics for psychologists. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963. Hurst, J. C., & Jensen, V. H. Personal growth: An ingre- dient in counselor education programs? Counselor Education and Supervision, 1968, 4, 12-17. Jourard, S. M. Counseling for authenticity. In C. E. Beck (Ed.), Guidelines for guidance. Dubuque: W. C. Brown, 1966. Klavetter, R. E., & Mogar, R. E. Stability and internal consistency of a measure of self-actualization. Psychological Reports, 1967, 44, 422-424. Knapp, R. R. Relationship of a measure of self-actualiza- tion to neuroticism and extraversion. Journal of Consulting Psycholo , 1965, 42, 168-172. Kramer, H. C. Effects of conditioning several responses in a group setting. Journal of Counseling‘ngchol— pgy, 1968, 4;, 58-62. Krumboltz, J. D. A behavioral approach to group counseling and therapy. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 1968, I, 3-18. Leib, J. W., & Snyder, W. U. Effects of group discussions on underachievement and self-actualization. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1967, 44, 282-285. Mahler, C. A. Group counseling. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1971, 42, 601-608. Maslow, A. H. Self-actualization and beyond. In J. F. T. Bugental (Ed.), Challenges of humanistic psychol- ggy, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1967: McClain, E. W. A program for increasing counselor self understanding. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 4, 296-302. McKinnon, D. W. Group counseling with student counselors. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 4, 1953200. 56 Muro, J. J. Some aspects of the group counseling practicum. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1968, 4, 371-378. Orton, J. W. Areas of focus in supervising counseling prac- ticum students in groups. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 44, 167-170. Prediger, D. J., & Baumann, R. R. Developmental group counseling: An outcome study. Journal of Coun- seling Psychology, 1970, 41, 527-533. Reddy, W. B. Sensitivity training as an integral phase of counselor education. Counselor Education and Super- vision, 1970, g, 110- 115. Rogers, C. R. The fully functioning person. ngchotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 1963, 4, 17-26. Seegars, J. E., & McDonald, R. L. The role of interaction groups in counselor education. Journal of Counsel- ing Psychology, 1963, 42, 156-162. Shostrom, E. L. A test for the measurement of self-actual- ization. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1964, 44, 207-218. Shostrom, E. L. Manual, Personal Orientation Inventory . San Diego: Education and Industrial Testing Service, 1966. Shostrom, E. L., & Knapp, R. R. The relationship of a mea- sure of self-actualization (POI) to a measure of pathology (MMPI) and to therapeutic growth. Ameri- can Journal of Psychotherapy, 1966, 20, 193- 252. Tuckman, B. W. Developmental sequence in small groups. ngchological Bulletin, 1965, 44, 384-399. Wirt, M., Betz, R., & Engle, K. The effects of group coun- seling on the self concepts of counselor candidates. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 8, 189- 200. Zimpfer, D. G. Some conceptual and research problems in group counseling. The School Counselor, 1968, 42, 326-333. APPENDIX A Typescript of Leader-Member Interactions 57 Typescript of Leader-Member Interactions And if I--if you could just convince these people, you know, "Forget it! I like you the way you are. You do what you do best." Being a housewife is sometimes just as artistic and just as great as being a college professor or having a degree. But, I think maybe Betty can identify with this. There are unique those certain people who really grind it into you. You know, and it is kind of traumatic and you remember those kinds of people. I guess, cause I was laying on the ground, and the floor wouldn't let go, and I wouldn't be . . . on the floor, I don't know how this thing is re- lated to trust. Whereas, like falling, I think, to me it seems more--we11, you'd better be there, I trust you to be there. If you're not, I'm going to hurt myself. O.K., want to do a falling exercise? That there's more effective ways for me to behave, like if I get very angry or, ah, very hurt I'll cry. And that doesn't help. I should do some- thing different from that. Because it--it doesn't help anything. And I've got to change that kind of . . . If I just sit there and cry rather than tell people what I feel . . . But that's hard to do if you feel like if you get angry that it's your fault, and you did something bad. The crying must mean guilt or something. . . I guess I feel like, I don't care who drops out or anything. It's just like you make up your own mind and the rest of us can get started. I guess that this is kind of difficult for me in the sense that, um, well in the sense that there was no intent or plan or reason for being together in a group providing this kind of treatment. So its kind of a hodge-podge of a lot of people put together. That was another thing I just remembered. What reinforced that was after the group then you went to Joe and began at a different level again. I said . . . you just reinforced my distrust. That's some feelings and reactions . . . I guess I have a feeling that, Dick, you have a lot of difficulty understanding how I can blast a guy and then--and still at the same time like him and be friends with him. 10. 11. 12. 58 And we don't even know what you're thinking--you're just keeping it in your head and twirling it round and round inside you. Yah. Well, I'm sure there's nothing there that . . It's either that, or you really don't just give a shit about us. You were severely stressing my ability to accept human difference, which I've always . . . I'd like some just personal things. Did I overdo it today? Yah, it was coming out in a game all the time. How is it supposed to be? Is it supposed to be not fun? - I guess that's something I never even considered. (Laughter) How about that. (I wonder) Did you find that you were able to look at yourself alot or were you still holding back in some way? I never hurt so much in my life as during those times. Since that time it has really gotten . . . I've got someone to care me and what more do I want. Did you marry your wife just to have a companion? I don't think it's--you're as much aware of it when you make that decision to start out with. I think it comes a little bit later. At least it did with me. Does it come--I guess I heard you say--like we could--you're speaking about work or . . . even- tually we'll freak out or something. I guess essentially you're pulling up, away from a group of friends, going somewhere else where you don't have to do this all again and make friends, make a place. This is the thing you would like to avoid? No one else ever has guts enough to say things like that. Pardon? No one else ever has guts enough to say things like this and probably may people have felt that way. But, you know, . . . I said I was feeling that way but it doesn't necessarily mean that's the way she was feeling. 13. 14. 15. 59 I guess it's the whole newness of the situation that gets me very upset and takes me a long time to establish myself. Especially if you don't approach, especially if you hold back. I don't understand why the other ones left. Um, I do--and I don't understand either. And that really kind of bothers me, too. You know, cause I don't want to--I want to contri- bute, but I just don't have--I don't know what to contribute. I think that's kind of putting yourself down. Total script contains 72 units of analysis. APPENDIX B Instructions to Judges 60 Instructions to Judges Here are 72 counselor responses from group counsel- ing sessions to be judged. Each member or group statement (M) is followed by the group leader response (L) made to that statement . If the group leader's response: Focuses on interpersonal or group feelings, Reflects the expression of affect (feeling), Would tend to elicit affective responses, or Would tend to cause examination of feelings, Then: Write "A" following the number corresponding to the member-leader statement being considered. If the group leader's response: Focuses on content produced by an individual or the group, Reflects the expression of cognitive material (content), Would tend to elicit cognitive responses, or Would tend to cause examination of content, Then: Write "C" following the number corresponding to the member-leader statement being considered. APPENDIX C Activity Evaluation Form 61 Activity Evaluation Form To what extent do you feel that your small group activity during spring Practicum was worthwhile to you? Much Somewhat Little Was it enjoyable? Yes No Was it a valuable learning experience? Yes NO Name of Group Leader ADM. PQM. Comments: APPENDIX D Mean POI Sub-scale Scores 62 Table A.1 Mean POI Sub-scale Scores Group 1 Group 2 Pre Post Pre Post Treatment 1 I 84.00 86.86 92.56 100.44 Tc 16.29 15.71 19.00 19.22 SAV 21.14 22.14 20.56 21.22 Ex 20.43 22.00 24.00 27.44 Fp 16.00 15.86 17.11 19.11 S 12.43 12.71 13.11 15.11 Sp 12.00 13.14 13.33 13.56 Sa 14.71 14.43 18.44 20.56 Nc 11.00 11.57 12.11 11.56 Sy 6.86 7.29 7.44 7.78 A 16.00 17.00 17.78 19.44 C 17.57 18.86 19.44 23.44 Treatment 2 I 89.71 97.29 90.00 99.78 Tc 15.14 17.57 16.89 18.11 SAV 20.86 21.29 19.89 22.33 Ex 23.14 25.14 23.78 27.56 Fp 17.57 18.29 17.00 19.11 S 12.71 13.57 13.11 14.89 Sr 11.71 12.86 12.11 13.11 Sa 17.00 18.29 16.56 18.33 Nc 12.14 13.14 12.89 13.33 Sy 7.29 7.71 7.00 8.22 A 16.71 17.86 18.00 19.33 C 19.14 21.71 20.00 21.56 63 Table A.1 (Cont.) Group 1 Group 2 Pre Post Pre Post Treatment 3 I 93.50 100.00 84.00 91.75 Tc 17.00 17.50 15.13 17.75 SAV 21.00 21.75 20.00 20.38 Ex 23.38 25.75 20.63 23.37 Fp 18.88 19.13 15.50 17.25 S 13.00 15.38 12.75 13.88 Sp 12.88 13.13 12.25 12.13 Sa 16.75 18.75 13.63 18.13 Nc 12.75 12.38 12.25 12.63 Sy 7.88 7.75 7.25 7.25 A 18.62 20.50 16.00 17.13 C 20.63 22.13 17.88 19.39 Treatment 4 I 86.44 89.78 93.83 101.83 Tc 17.11 17.44 18.67 19.33 SAV 20.44 21.33 20.67 22.00 Ex 22.89 25.00 24.00 27.50 Fr 17.00 16.44 17.17 18.83 S 12.44 13.56 14.00 14.67 Sp 11.44 11.44 14.67 13.33 Sa 16.33 16.44 18.00 19.33 No 11.56 12.00 12.50 12.00 Sy 7.67 7.22 7.67 8.17 A 14.56 16.22 19.17 20.50 C 20.00 20.00 20.00 23.50 64 Table A.1 (Cont.) Group 1 Group 2 Pre Post Pre Post Treatment 5 I 90.80 94.00 86.75 93.25 Tc 18.00 17.20 19.75 19.50 SAV 21.00 20.80 20.75 20.50 Ex 24.60 25.00 21.50 23.25 Fr 15.40 16.60 17.25 17.00 S 13.60 14.40 12.50 12.75 Sr 12.60 12.80 13.50 13.50 Sa 19.00 19.00 15.50 19.00 Nc 11.00 12.80 12.25 12.25 Sy 7.00 7.00 7.75 8.00 A 15.80 17.40 15.00 17.00 C 22.20 21.60 19.25 20.00 APPENDIX E Mean, Variance, and Standard Deviation of All Variables 65 Table A2. Mean, Variance, and Standard Deviation of 'All’Variables. Variable Mean Variance S.D. I 95.65 41.61 6.45 Tc 17.89 4.38 2.09 SAV 21.43 3.74 1.94 Ex 25.36 4.76 2.19 Fr 17.86 3.70 1.92 S 14.18 2.99 1.73 Sr 12.85 2.71 1.65 Sa 18.18 6.42 2.53 Nc 12.36 3.07 1.75 Sy 7.64 1.31 1.14 A 18.31 4.61 2.15 C 21.25 5.61 2.37 D/T 43.79 129.34 11.37 MICHIGRN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 31293006427136