-—v—_ -—-‘ v.- —————— REMOTE TORAGE $ ' [son '- Date " IIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 1293 00646 4097 LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled CONFLICT RESOLUTION BEHAVIOR IN DATING RELATIONSHIPS: _ TYPOLOGIES AND PREDICTORS presented by w. John Curtis has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. degree in Psychology /," ‘ { Major {)rofessor / Ga y Sto‘liak November 4 , 1987 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution REMOTE STORAGE PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. I DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE AMI; 341 12038 i“ 2/17 20:: Blue FORMS/DateDueForms_2017.indd - 99.5 ‘Ct‘ fl“ (II \o CONFLICT RESOLUTION BEHAVIOR IN DATING RELATIONSHIPS: TYPOLOGIES AND PREDICTORS By w. John Curtis A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1 987 ABSTRACT CONFLICT RESOLUTION BEHAVIOR IN DATING RELATIONSHIPS: TYPOLOGIES AND PREDICTORS By W. John Curtis A sample of 671 undergraduates was surveyed with the Conflict Resolution and Abuse Questionnaire (CRAQ) concerning the types of conflict resolution behaviors they utilized in their current dating relationship. Respondents were also surveyed concerning the characteristics of their dating relationship, the types of conflict resolution behaviors used by their partner, and the history of violence in their family of origin. A factor analysis of the CRAQ items yielded seven factors representing seven types of conflict resolution behavior. A cluster analysis of the subjects resulted in a typology of conflict resolution behavior. Length of and depth of committment in the relationship. amount of violence received from the dating partner, and violence in the family of origin were used to predict conflict resolution behavior. The strongest finding of the study indicated that violence in dming relationships is a reciporical phenommeneon. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Throughout this process of writing a Master's Thesis, many people have been helpful, supportive, and loving in their own way. Gary Stollak, to me you have been a friend, mentor, teacher, and philosopher. Your words and your courage to apply philosophy and theory to your everyday life have more often than not been an inspiration to me. In my struggle to begin to comprehend human behavior and interaction, you have acted as a guide to the at once unknown and often discouragingly unknowable realm of human existence. Your belief in the ultimate and attainable goal of knowledge has motivated and encouraged me when I felt unable to go any further. You have invited me into your home and l have felt at home there. We have talked about Asimov and Clarke. For all of this I am eternally grateful, and wherever and whenever, I will often and fondly remember. Larry Messe, you helped and helped, unselfishly and patiently giving time, knowledge, and advice. Especially when it counted and was needed. Thank you. Susan Frank, thank you for the very helpful comments and insights on the analyses. They put me on the right track. Gene Maguin. A friend and colleague. One who has helped me in immeasurable ways to begin to understand and tame the computer and statistics beasts. Also, one who always listened to whatever complaints I might have had about anything, and often provided words of wisdom concerning life and this violence issue we have gotten ourselves into. The innumerable conversations and ideas were both inspirational and entertaining, to say the least. And for all the glasses of water. Thank you. Amy, thank you for love, understanding, and comfort. You inspired in ways you never knew. Melvin and Nina, two wonderful people from Virginia, without whom I would not have begun this journey, thank you. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page CONFLICT RESOLUTION BEHAVIOR IN DATING RELATIONSHIPS. TYPOLOGIES AND PREDICTORS . . . . . 1 INTRODUCTION . . . . . . 1 Measurement of Conflict Resolution Behavior . . . . . 4 The CRAO. . . . . 8 Reciprocity and Sex Differences in Conflict Resolution Behavior . . . . 10 Violence in the Family of Origin . . . 13 Conflict Resolution and Duration and Type of Dating Relationship . . . 17 HYPOTHESES . . . . . . 19 METHOD . . . . . . 20 Subjects . . . . . . 20 Instrument . . . . . . 23 Derivation of Scales . . . . . 26 Procedure . . . . . . 27 RESULTS . . . . . . 28 Factor Analysis . . . . . 28 Cluster Analysis . . . . 31 Multivariate Analysis of Variance . . . 35 Individual Tests of Predictors . . . 35 Summary of Cluster Differences . . . 37 Discriminant Function Analysis . . . 37 Discriminating Variables . . . . 40 Classification Results . . . . 43 DISCUSSION APPENDICES A: FF???) [9999? Conflict Resolution and Abuse Questionnaire . Conflict Tactics Scale (Form A) Conflict Tactics Scale (Form N) Index of Spouse Abuse . Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 . . Scree Plot of Eigenvalues Items and Item Loadings of CRAO Factors Plot of Group Centroids on Two Discriminant Functions REFERENCES vi 8 74 75 76 77 87 92 93 Table 10. 11. 12. LIST OF TABLES Length of respondents' current or most recent dating relationship Depth of comittment of respondents' current or most recent dating relationship Frequency of endorsement of parent-child violence items Frequency of endorsement of parent-parent violence items . Coefficient alphas of subscales Alpha coefficients for CRAO factors Pearson correlations between factors of the CRAO Cluster populations Cluster means on seven CRAO factors Cluster standard deviations on seven CRAO factors Means and F-ratios of dependent variables for each group Characteristics of discriminant functions derived vii Page 21 22 23 27 30 31 34 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 21. 24. Mean discriminant functions evaluated at group means (group centroids) Standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients . Correlations between discriminant functions and discriminating variables Discriminant function classification results . . Frequency of responses to CRAO items 1-67 . . Appropriate/Generic factor of the CRAO Moderate violence factor of the CRAO Avoid factor of the CRAO Severe violence factor of the CRAO Distraction factor of‘the CRAO . Verbal Attack/Coercion factor of the CRAO Symbolic/Indirect violence factor of the CRAO . viii 39 41 42 77 87 88 89 89 90 90 91 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Scree plot of eigenvalues . . . . 86 2. Plot of four group centroids on two discriminant functions . . . . 92 INTRODUCTION A large body of work has focused on the the content and dynamics of conflict resolution between intimates (e.g., Gottman, 1979; Peterson, 1983). This work has largely involved assessment of marital conflict and coding of couples discussing one or more areas of conflict. As expected, research (e.g., Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980; Yllo & Straus, 1981; Makepeace, 1981; Sigelman, Sony, 8: Vlfiles, 1984) has demonstrated that both married and dating couples utilize physically coercive/violent behavior(s) in the resolution of conflict within their relationships. Straus, et al. (1980), in their survey of 2143 American families, found that approximately 17% reported that they had experienced at least one act of spousal violence in the last year. Similarly, in his ground breaking survey of violence among college dating couples, Makepeace (1981) found that 23% of those surveyed reported they had experienced at least one act of violence in a dating relationship. This literature has, until recently, pn'marily been aimed at establishing that such violence occurs. Only recently has work begun to establish some of the basic correlates of violent behavior in intimate relationships (e.g., Arias & O'Leary, 1984; Lane & Gwartney~Gibbs, 1985). To date, no research has attempted to integrate the findings from the marital/premarital violence literatures and the more general area of marital conflict assessment. It is reasonable to assume, given the prevalence of marital violence, that some couples who have participated in studies assessing marital conflict (e.g., Gottman, 1979) also experience violence in their relationships. They simply have not been asked, and will not likely be demonstrating this behavior in front of an observer or video camera. However, 1 2 it seems probable that violent conflict resolution behavior must certainly be exerting a powerful onfluence on the styles of conflict resolution exhibited in such studies, and in fact has been demonstrated to have a dramatic impact on the functioning and structure of the marital relationship (Walker, 1979). The present study assessed the kind and frequency of conflict resolution behaviors occurring in dating relationships and of some of the correlates of these behaviors. The study assessed the frequency of a broad range of conflict resolution behaviors, including physically violent and verbally and emotionally abusive behaviors, as well as positive, constructive behaviors. These behaviors were assessed by the Conflict Resolution and Abuse Questionnaire (CRAO) (see Appendix A), an instrument developed to determine how often a wide range of specific behaviors are exhibited and experienced during the subjects' current or most recent (if not currently involved) dating relationship. Specifically, each subject was asked to report on the frequency with which slhe exhibited such behaviors, and also how often slhe was the recipient of these same behaviors. The first phase of the study involved a factor analysis of the conflict resolution items from the CRAO. The items from the CRAO represent a broad range of conflict resolution behaviors, and were found to be grouped into factors, each of which represents one specific style or class of conflict resolution behavior. The development of such factors provided an opportunity to investigate relationships among several different classes of violent and non-violent conflict resolution behaviors, and the association of various ' conflict resolution behaviors with a number of hypothesized predictors of violent conflict resolution behavior. The first area investigated was the respondents' reported conflict resolution behavior. Specifically, what types, if any, of non-violent methods of conflict resolution do subjects use who also report using violence to resolve conflicts ? Do these who exhibit violent behaviors to resolve conflict also exhibit fewer non-violent, or prosocial conflict resolution behaviors? Also, are the. non-violent behaviors exhibited by respondents who also report abuse 3 ciifferent from those methods of non-violent conflict resolution reported by overall non-violent, or less violent subjects ? Secondly. conflict resolution behaviors were related to the reported level of violence in the family of origin. The first question investigated in this context concerned the relationship between being the recipient of violence as a child (perpetrated by parent(s)) and the reported conflict resolution behavior in the respondents' current or most recent dating relationship. Does being the recipient of violence as a child relate to being more or less likely to be the perpetrator and/or recipient of maladaptive, or even violent conflict resolution behavior in a dating relationship ? Further, information concerning physical violence witnessed between the respondents' parents was compared with the reported conflict resolution behaviors in the respondents' dating relationship. Is witnessing violence between parents associated with violent conflict resolution behavior in dating relationships ? If so, is one more likely to be a recipient and/or a perpetrator of maladaptive or violent conflict resolution behavior ? A third area investigated was the relationship between the duration and level of committment of the respondent's relationship and the type of conflict resolution behavior reported. Do those involved in a more committed type of relationship (i.e., dating exclusively or discussing marriage) report different conflict resolution behaviors than those in uncommitted relationships 7 Further. does the amount of time one has spent in the relationship relate to the type of conflict resolution behavior utilized in the relationship ? This area of inquiry may begin to shed some light on the development of conflict resolution behaviors (within the temporal context of a dating relationship). Finally, a fourth area investigated was the extent of reciprocity of conflict resolution behaviors. The questions addressed here are: Is being the recipient of one type or types of conflict resolution behavior(s) asssociated with using those same types of behaviors, or are there subjects who appear to utilize a set of conflict resolution behaviors regardless of the conflict resolution behavior experienced by them. 4 Each of these four areas were examined as predictors of different types or categories of conflict resolution behaviors. Also, sex differences within each of these predictors were investigated. II III: II'IB ll' El' Previous investigators have detailed the relationship between different intensities and frequencies of reported interspousal violence (e.g., Straus, 1979), and the relationship of this violence with other kinds of conflict resolution. However, this work has been based largely upon a 14 or19 item measure of conflict resolution strategies developed by Straus (1979) called the Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS) (See Appendix B ). This instnrment was developed by Straus as a technique to measure the ways in which family members resolve conflicts. The conflict resolution tactics to be measured by the CTS were chosen initially as a means to test the hypothesis that greater use of intellectual andreasoning tactics for resolving conflict will result in less use of verbally or physically aggressive methods of conflict resolution between family members (Straus, 1974). The items which comprise the original CTS (as well as the revised form) were designed to fit into three categories of conflict resolution: 1) the use of rational discussion and reasoning (called the Reasoning scale by Straus,1979). 2) the use of verbal and nonverbal acts which symbolically hurt the other, or the use of threats to harm the other (called the Verbal Aggression scale), and 3) the use of physical force against another person as a means of conflict resolution (called the Violence scale). The original CTS (Form A) consists of a 14 item list of actions which might occur in conflicts. The items start with those low in coerciveness and become gradually more coercive and destructive towards the end of the list. The respondent is asked to indicate the number of times each action occurred during the past year, ranging from "Never" to "More than 20 times". The CTS is designed to measure conflict resolution tactics used by all nuclear family members (e.g., between siblings, 5 parent and child, etc.), as well as between spouses. A revised version of the CTS (Form N) (see Appendix C) was developed for a national interview survey (Straus et al., 1980). This scale has 19 rather than 14 items. Minor revisions include a greater focus on the Verbal Aggression and Wolence scales (via the dropping of one Reasoning item and adding two Verbal Aggression and three Vrolence items), changes in the wording of some of the items, a place to record if the act ever happened was added, and the reponse category range was increased from 0 to 5 to O to 6. Straus' (1979) factor analysis of the CTS confirmed the three major conflict tactics included in the CTS. The factor analyses were computed for interspousal conflict resolution using the sample described in Straus (1974) with N-385, and replicated with the sample described in Straus et al. (1980) with N-2143. The first sample, using Form A of the CTS produced three factors, each of which identifies with one of the three conceptual Conflict Tactic (CT) Scales. With the second sample, using a different form (Form N), Straus obtained four factors. The extra factor corresponds to two new items on this form not included in Form A, specifically, the use of a knife or a gun. Thus, this additional factor represents a separate dimension, life threatening violence. The factor stnrcture for husband to wife violence is very similar to the structure obtained for wife to husband violence. Analyses of the national sample's responses found that of subjects who reported engaging in little or no verbal aggression, less than 0.5% also reported physical violence. Of those couples who reported a low to moderate level of verbal aggression, 5% also reported physical violence. Nineteen percent of couples in the upper middle range of verbal aggression reported violence, and 56% of these couples who were in the top 25% in verbal aggression reported physical violence. These results seem to indicate that the use of verbal aggression in conflict resolution is associated with the use of physical violence. However, given the limitations of Straus' CTS (there are only three items which could be. considered verbally aggressive behaviors), it is impossible to determine what 6 other, if any, nonviolent strategies violent couples employ when attempting to resolve conflict. Further, Straus (1979) used an orthogonal rotation for his factor analysis, and thus sought to minimize the possibility of interfactor correlations. Straus (1979) does not report which, if any, of the Reasoning scale items were used by violent versus nonviolent subjects, and only mentions that the reasoning items form one of the four factors derived by the factor analysis. Straus (1979) also reports on the internal consistency reliability of the CTS. For Form A, item-total correlations were computed for each of the three subscales. For the Reasoning scale, the mean item-total correlation (r) for the husbands was .74 and .70 for the wives. On the Verbal Aggression scale, mean item-total correlations for husbands and wives were .73 and .70, respectively. For the Violence scale, the mean correlations for husbands were .87 and .88 for wives. ' For Form N, Straus (1979) computed‘the Alpha coefficient of reliability for the three subscales. For husband to wife, Coefficient alphas for the Reasoning, Verbal Aggression and Violence sacles were .50, .80, and .83 respectively. For wife to husband, coefficient alphas for the three scales were .51, .88, and .88. These coefficients are reasonably high for the Verbal Aggression and Violence scales. However, the low coefficients for the Reasoning scale are a function of the small number of items (three) which make up the Reasoning scale (Straus, 1979). The CTS Form A items were included in a list of 23 conflict areas used by Jorgensen (1977) in a study investigating the amount of marital distress in marriages where the partners are of differing social class background. Jorgensen does not specify what the conflict areas were. The questionnaire consisted of asking the respondents how often (from never to always) they and their spouses engaged in each behavior listed during a marital conflict. In a factor analysis performed on the items of this scale, Jorgensen (1977) found three factors, corresponding to three levels of intensity in marital conflict: 1) a high intensity factor, which consists entirely of items involving physical assault 7 and force, 2) a medium intensity factor, which includes acts of verbal or symbolic aggression (eg., insulting the other or breaking something), and 3) a low intensity factor, which consists of items involving reasoning, empathy, and providing information. Hudson and McIntosh (1981 ), in an attempt to expand upon Straus' CTS developed the Index of Spouse Abuse (ISA) (see Appendix D). This is a 30-item scale designed to measure the severity of physical (ISA-P) and nonphysical (ISA-NP) abuse that is inflicted upon a woman by her spouse or partner. The investigators derived a complex scoring system utilizing item weights to account for the fact that some of the items represent very serious forms of abuse and others do not. Their scoring formula produces scores that range between 0 and 100. Hudson and McIntosh used the ISA to study 398 graduate and undergraduate female college students who were either married or involved in an ongoing dating relationship with a male partner. The purpose of this study was to confirm the presence of two dimensions of abuse which were purportedly measured by the ISA; physical and nonphysical. Thus, the authors performed a factor analysis of the responses of this sample. The 25 items of the Index of Marital Satisfaction (IMS) (which was also administered to this sample) were also included in the factor analysis as a check on the factorial validity of the ISA items. Thus, it was expected that three factors would emerge: one for the IMS, and two for the ISA, and if the IMS and the ISA were conceptually distinct, that the IMS items should lead highest on the first factor, and the ISA items should load highest on the second and third factors. This procedure was intended as a check on whether or not the ISA is merely an alternate form of measuring marital discord. Three clear factors emerged from the factor analysis, which confirmed that the ISA is a measure distinct from marital discord as measured by the IMS. Specifically, all of the IMS items loaded highest on the first factor, and none of the ISA items had their highest loading on this factor. The physical abuse items from the ISA loaded on the second factor at .50 or better, and none had loadings of this magnitude on the other two factors. Finally, nearly all of the nonphysical 8 abuse items loaded at .40 or better on factor three. Reliabilities of the two ISA subscales were examined utilizing coefficient alpha as an estimator. The ISA-P and ISA-NP subscale reliabilities averaged across two independent samples were approximately .92 and .93, respectively. These coefficients are high, and support the claim of Hudson and McIntosh (1981) that the ISA subscales are unidimensional measures. MEAD The CRAO is an instrument designed to be more comprehensive than either the CTS or the ISA in its assessment of a much wider range of conflict resolution and interpersonal (including sexual) behaviors. Specifically, the CTS is limited to the assessment of a narrow range of possible behaviors within conflict resolution. It lists a total of 13 violent and 5 nonviolent conflict resolution behaviors. The CRAO includes 67 items which are descriptions of a wide range of behaviors that might occur when a couple is confronted with conflict. All of the items from Straus' CTS are Included within the 67 conflict resolution items of the CRAO. However, unlike on the CTS, each item represents only one behavior. For example, CTS (Form N) item n. reads “Kicked, bit, or hit with a fist“. On the CRAO, each of these behaviors, kicking, biting, and hitting with a fist, is a separate item. On the CTS, if a person endorses item n., there is no way to know which of the violent behaviors they are referring to. The distinction between these behaviors may not be necessary, as each is simply a violent, assaultive act that could produce tissue damage. However, there is no evidence to indicate that a person who hits is also likely to bite and or kick when resolving conflict. Also, one who bites may not hit with their fist during a conflict. The use of the CTS does not allow the distinction between which of the act or acts the respondent is referring to. With the CRAO, since each item represents only one behavior, there is no 9 uncertainty about the behavior of interest. Another advantage of having an expanded number of conflict resolution items is that it enables the measurement of a wider variety of both negative and positive conflict resolution tactics. Straus (1979) includes three general forms of conflict resolution on the CTS: reasoning, verbal and nonverbal acts which symbolically hurt the other, and physical force. However, these three categories of conflict tactics do not represent a broad range of human behavior during conflict, and are certainly not exhaustive. For example, a person may also attempt to avoid conflict (e.g., put off talking about a problem), deny conflict (e.g., not accept that there is a problem), bring in a third party to help resolve the conflict, or attempt to distract the other from the issue, to mention a few alternatives. Also, even within Straus' three categories distinctions could be made between various forms of reasoning (e.g., discussing the issue calmly vs. suggesting possible solutions), and different types of verbal assault (e.g., an . insult vs. yelling), and although Straus distinguishes between extreme violence (using a knife or a gun) and other forms of physical assault, there is no sharp distinction between different violent behaviors, for example, varying intensities of violence (e.g., squeezing someone's arm, pulling hair, or slapping a lower body part vs. slapping the face, hitting someone with an object vs. choking the person, etc.). Most importantly, Straus does not (and given the brevity and generality of the CTS is not able to) delineate the relationship(s) between the different reported conflict tactics. For example, if a respondent indicates on the CTS that he or she is the perpetrator of a violent act, such as hitting with a fist, Straus generally does not report what other violent or nonviolent acts he or she uses. Hudson and McIntosh (1981) overcome some of the shortcomings of the CTS with their ISA instrument. However, the ISA items are not presented in the context of conflict resolution. The respondent is simply instructed to report the degree of abuse experienced in her (the ISA is only intended for female 1 0 respondents) relationship with her partner. Thus, a direct comparison with the CTS is not possible, as the two questionnaires elicit different information. The ISA is a list of physically and nonphysically (psychologically and sexually) abusive acts which presents a wider range of behaviors than the CTS. However, there is no way to determine in what context the reported acts took place (i.e., during a conflict, or with no apparent reason, etc.). Also, there are no positive or nonabusive acts included as items on the ISA. Therefore, the factor analysis could only reveal two factors, physical and nonphysical abuse, which provide little information beyond the validation of scale items. 8' 'I IS D'II i8 III 8 II' Bl' An important issue to be addressed in any inquiry concerning behavior ocurring between two people Is the extent of reciprocity of that behavior. For the most part, research in the area of violence between spouses has focused upon the one-way nature of such interaction (i.e., male assaulting female) (e.g., Pagelow, 1981; Roy, 1977; Gelles, 1974; Walker, 1979). However, recent surveys of married and dating couples have indicated that violent interactions between intimate heterosexual couples is indeed reciprocal (e.g., Straus et al.,1980; Steinmetz, 1980; Sigelman at al.,1984; Lane 8: Gwartney-Gibbs, 1985). In fact, there seem to be no significant sex differences in the reported use of violence in both married and dating couples. However, in order to determine the reciprocity of abuse, one must actually specify that the interaction is indeed reciprocal within each couple, rather than simply indicate that there are no sex differences in the behavior across subjects. In their survey of 2143 families using the CTS, Straus, et al. (1980) found no statistical difference between the rates of violence reported by men and women. The only differences revealed indicated that twice as many wives as husbands threw something at their spouse. The rate for kicking, biting, and 1 1 hitting with an object is also higher for wives than for husbands. Husbands had higher rates for pushing, shoving, slapping, beefing up, and using a knife or a gun. Thus, even though overall violence rates are similar for husbands and wives, it appears from these data that husbands are overall more likely to use the most severe and potentially life threatening conflict resolution tactics than wives. Straus et al. (1980) report that although most of the violence towards husbands by women in this sample seems to be committed in self-defense, nearly 30 percent of the women committed violent acts that could not be classified as such. Data from several other studies of violent marriages further reflect this phenommenon of reciprocity in abuse (Steinmetz, 1977; Steinmetz, 1980). However, Steinmetz notes that when one focuses on more severe violence, differences between husbands and wives do emerge. Steinmetz (1977) provides data based on police records and a random sample of families which estimates that 7 percent of the wives and 0.6 percent of the husbands would be victims of physical abuse resulting in serious tissue damage or even death by their spouses. In a survey study of violence in dating relationships, Sigelman et al. (1984) found no overall difference between sexes in the commission of at least one form of physical aggression. They found that women, in fact, show a greater tendency to engage in some of the more serious forms of violence (e.g., using a weapon). Also, the men in their sample reported having been the recipient of at least one such serious act more frequently than women. Sigelman et al. (1984) also found that physical violence in dating relationships was usually mutual as well. They report that for both men and women, having physically abused a partner in some way was strongly related to having been physically abused in some way; 47.3 percent of the men in their sample and 40.2 percent of the women reported both abusing partners and being abused themselves. On the other hand, 34.5 percent of the men and 40.5 percent of the women reported being neither perpetrators nor recipients of violence. 1 2 From a sample of 371 college undergraduates surveyed concerning violence in their relationships. Laner and Thompson (1982) and Laner (1983) also reported no sex differences in violence between dating couples. Although not reported, on the basis of the frequency data broken down by sex, there appears to be no large differences between rates of violence reported by men and women. Further, Laner (1983) found that 67 percent of the men and 48 percent of the women stated that violent events in the relationship were mutual. ' Lane and Gwartney-Gibbs (1985) using data from a survey of 325 college undergraduates also confirm the finding that violence between dating couples is inflicted and experienced equally by men and women. However, more males reported having perpetrated more serious forms of assault against women (e.g., using a weapon or causing severe tissue damage). These authors further found that males were more likely to report having experienced violence on multiple occasions with multiple partners, wheras most of the women in the sample reported having expereineced violence only once with one person. A survey of 461 college students by Bernard and Bemard (1983) also supports the finding of mutuality of violence. On the other hand, Makepeace (1983) found, in his survey of 244 college undergraduates, that men were more likely than women to report having used some type of violence in the context of a dating relationship. Further, the rate of using severe violence against a partner was found to be 2.5 times higher for males than for females. However, when examining the rate of reported receptive violence, there were no significant sex differences. Stats 8. Pirog-Good (1987), in a survey of 505 college undergraduates, found that women are more likely than men to receive violence in a dating relationship. Howevr, women in their sample reported using the same amount of violence as the men. The scoring system used by these investigators did not permit analysis of sex cfifferences across various levels of severity of violence. It is apparent that verbal and physical assault between married or 1 3 unmarried couples is equally likely to be perpetrated and/or received by men and women, although there is some evidence indicating that this may not be true for all forms of violence, and there may be sex differences in using versus receiving violence. All of the studies cited above used the CTS or a modification thereof in the gathering of their data Thus, the subjects involved were reporting on a narrow range of primarily violent behaviors used to resolve conflict. However, given the limitations of the CTS, it is impossible to determine the mutuality of other nonviolent or maladaptive forms of conflict resolution, and how these other forms of conflict resolution might be related to violent conflict resolution and related sex differences. For quite some time investigators have demonstrated a link between violence and aggression in the home and violence perpetrated by children (e.g., McCord, McCord, and Howard, 1961; Bandura, Ross and Ross, 1961; McCord, McCord, and Howard,1963; Bandura and Walters, 1963; Bandura,1973; Patterson, 1976). Early investigators established that children who are raised in homes where they are consistently exposed to aggressive and violent acts by their parents are more likely than children in nonaggressive homes to be aggressive and violent towards peers. Bandura (1973) hypothesized that this transmission of aggressive behavior from parents to children takes place through social learning; children learn through the observation of their parents' aggressive behaviors and the reinforcements their parents' receive as a result of such behavior. Early investigators in the area of spouse abuse (e.g., Gelles,1974; Owens and Straus, 1975; Carroll, 1977) observed that both the victims and perpetrators of conjugal violence were more likely to have experienced violence in their families' of origin, both as witnesses and victims. In a 1 4 nationwide survey conducted by the President's Commission on the Causes and Prevention of violence, Owens and Straus (1975) found that adults' approval of violence was as highly correlated with being a victim of violence as a child as was committing a violent act as an adult. Carroll (1977) found a clear relationship between the severity of parental punishment and reports of family violence in his sample of 375 families. Straus et al. (1980) also gathered extensive data on the respondents' history of violence in their family of origin. Men who had seen their parents physically attack each other were almost three times more likely to have been violent toward their wives during the past year; 35% of the sons of violent parents versus 10.7% of the sons of non-violent parents reported using violence against their wives. The statistics for women parallelled those of the men; 26.7% of the women who had observed their parents engage in violence against each other reported using violence against their husbands, as compared to 8.9% of the daughters of non-violent parents. Straus et al. (1980) also reported that, in general, the more punishment received as a child, the greater the rate of violence in marriage 15 or more years later. Specifically, people whose parents did not hit them as teen-agers had the lowest rate of violent marriages. Approximately one out of four of those who reported receiving physical punishment as a child also reported being violent toward his or her spouse during the survey year. Of the men who were not physically punished as teenagers, only two out of a hundred reported violence toward their spouse. Straus, et al. (1980) report that this same low rate also applies to assault of husbands by women who had not been hit as teen-agers. They also hypothesized that subjects who both witnessed their parents being violent toward each other and experienced physical punishment themselves would report the most conjugal violence, more than either group by itself. This hypothesis was also confirmed. The group having experienced both types of violence during childhood had the highest rate of violence in their own marriages (25% reported conjugal violence). 1 5 In a survey of violence in dating relationships Bernard and Bernard (1983) found that in their sample 73% of the abusive males had either experienced or observed abuse in their families of origin, while only 32% of the nonabusive males had either experienced or observed abuse in their families of origin. Of the abusive females in the sample, 50% had either experienced or observed violence in their families of origin, while 23% of the nonabusive females had observed or witnessed abuse in their families of origin. They further reported that there was no relationship between the type of exposure a child had to abuse in the family of origin and whether or not they became abusive themselves. Laner and Thompson (1982) reported similar findings concerning childhood exposure to violence and violence in a dating relationship. It was found that 60% of the women and 67% of the men who had experienced childhood violence were also the recipients of abuse in courtship. However, for both sexes, those who had not experienced childhood violence still “had about an even chance” (p.236) of experiencing violence in a dating relationship. Also, it was found that of those subjects who had experienced violence during childhood, approximately 50% reported inflicting violent behaviors in dating relationships. However, 33% of those who did not report childhood violence also reported inflicting violence in a dating relationship. However, Laner and Thompson (1982) did not collect data concerning the extent of violence between parents witnessed by subjects. The authors found that being the recipient of violence as a child did increase the Iikliehood of receiving or perpetrating violence in a dating relationship, although a significant proportion of those who did not receive violence in childhood also reported violent experiences in dating. However, they note that this association may have been stronger had data concerning witnessing of violence been gathered. They provided no evidence other than frequency statistics to support any of these claims. In a survey of 505 college undergraduates, Stats 8: Pirog-Good (1987) utilized a probit model to estimate both using and receiving violence in a 1 6 dating relationship for males and females. These investigators found that experiencing violence in childhood was a significant determinant of receiving violence for males in a dating relationship. However, this variable did not significantly increase the Iikliehood of either males or females using violence, or of females receiving violence in a dating relationship. Further, witnessing violence as a child did not have a significant effect on using or receiving violence for men or women. Stats & Pirog-Good concluded that these historical factors may be more likely to explain violence in more serious relationships. In separate discriminant analyses for college males and females, Sigelman et al. (1984) linked involvement in abuse to having been abused as a child, but only found a significant relationship for the women in their sample. Further, there was no significant relationship between having witnessed parental violence and dating violence for either males or females in their sample. Thus, with data subjected to a somewhat more sophisticated multivariate statistical analysis (i.e., a discriminant analysis), there is not as clear a relationship between childhood history of violence and courtship violence as was previously reported in other studies. The reason for this ciscrepancy with other studies is not clear. Sigelman et al. (1984) suggest that their correlational and discrrninant analyses offered a ”methodological waming' about research on this sensitive topic: the possibility that socially desirable responding, especially among women, leads to underreporting of experience with abusive behavior. However, these authors do not make it clear why socially desirable responding would result in low and/or nonsignificant correlations in such data, other than implying that underreporting could lead to these statistical problems. Further, exposure to violence as a child might not be as important a factor in violence in dating relationships for men as it is for women due to sex differences in socialization. Boys tend to be socialized in a more aggressive fashion than girls (e.g., being exposed to more physical play by their fathers, being given toy guns, (3. l. Joe and Rambo dolls to play with, etc.), regardless 1 7 of whether or not they are physically abused by their parents or they observe their parents being violent toward each other. Evidence indicates that overall boys are more aggressive than girls (e.g., Hartup,1977; Maccoby and Jacklin, 1980). Thus, boys may not require the direct exposure to childhood abuse or parental abuse to become involved in dating violence as a young adult. On the other hand, women, who are not usually socialized In an aggressive way, may not be prone to courtship violence unless there is a history of violence in their childhood. It is apparent that early studies of marital and dating violence found strong evidence for a link between family violence in chioldhood and the use of violence in a current intimate relationship. However, more recent studies utilizing multivariate statistical techniques have demonstrated that this relationships is not as strong as previously believed, and in one study (Stats & Pirog-Good, 1987), history of violence was not associated with the use of violence for either sex. I: II'IB II' III I’ II IDI' BII' l' Three studies investigating dating violence have examined the relationship between the duration and level of committment of relationships and the type of conflict resolution behavior in the relationship. Laner and Thompson (1982) found that regardless of the type of violent conflict resolution behavior, relationships defined as serious and meaningful (exact definition not provided by the authors) showed higher rates of abusive and aggressive conflict resolution behaviors than those defined as casual. Sigelman at al. (1984) found that 74% of the relationships with abusive conflict resolution behaviors were characterized as beyond the ”casual dating" stage and into either the “serious dating” stage or beyond (i.e., engagement or cohabitation) when the violence began. Stats 8 Pirog-Good (1987) found that length of relationship was a 1 8 significant determinant for using and receiving violence in a dating relationship for males, but was not associated with using or receiving violence for females. Measuring degree of seriousness by the combination of frequent dating and lengthy relationships, Stats & Pirog-Good (1987) also found that being involved in a more serious relationship was associated with using violence for males. However, they did not find this direct relationship between length and depth of relationship and using violence for females. Other studies of dating violence have not included reports on the level of involvement or duration of the relationships where maladapive conflict resolution behaviors are reported. Studies of violent conflict resolution behavior between marital partners by definition only include couples at one level of stated relationship commitment. Further, these studies generally do not ask respondents about violence that might have occurred in this or previous relationships before the marriage. Thus, very little information exists concaming the evolution of conflict resolution behavior in heterosexual relationships, and the different behavioral characteristics of conflict resolution at varying levels of commitment to and duration of the relationship. Hypotheses . Reports of being the recipient of violence as a child will be associated with reports of current or recent coercive conflict resolution behavior. . Reports of witnessing violence between parents as a child will be associated with reports of current or recent coercive conflict resolution behavior. . Those who report a longer duration relationship will report using more coercive conflict resolution behaviors. . Those who report a more committed relationship will report using more coercive conflict resolution behaviors. . Those who report being the recipient of current or recent coercive conflict resolution behaviors will also report using coercive conflict resolution behaviors. 19 Method Subjects. A sample of 671 undergraduates completed the CRAO. The respondents were students in introductory psychology classes at Michigan State University who participated in the study in exchange for extra credit points. Of these,10 indicated that they were married, and were dropped from the final samme, which was 661. The sample was 54% female and 46% male. The sample ranged in age from 16 to 44, with a mean age of 19.6 years. Racially, the sample was 90% white, 8% black, and approximately 2% Hispanic, Asian, and others. Fifty nine percent of the respondents reported that they were currently in a relationship; the 41% of the sample who reported they were not currently in a relationship completed the CRAO based on their most recent relationship. Differences between these two groups on the hypothesized variables were tested via Chi Square, and none were significant. Seventy three and nine tenths percent of the respondents reported living with their biological mother and father during high school, 8.1% reported living with their biological mother and a step-father, 2.9% reported living with their biological father and step-mother, 12.4% reported living in single mother homes, 0.9% reported living in single father homes, and 1.8% reported living with adoptive parents. Twenty seven and seven tenths percent of the respondents reported that their fathers had graduated from college and 28.9% reported that their fathers had gone to graduate or professional school. Sixteen and seven tenths percent of the fathers had reportedly graduated from high school only, and 5.0% had only completed some high school. For mothers, 28.3% of the respondents reported that their mothers had graduated from college and 14% reported that their mothers had gone to graduate or professional school. Twenty eight and three tenths percent of the mothers had graduated from high school only, and 3.8% had only completed some high school. Seventy six and nine tenths percent of the respondents reported that their fathers held white collar or professional jobs, while 15.7% reported their fathers 2O 21 holding blue collar jobs (e.g., factory workers). Fifty seven and four tenths percent of the respondents reported that their mothers held white collar or professional jobs,10.3% reported their mothers with blue collar jobs, and 28.4% of the respondents reported that their mother was a homemaker only. The proportions of respondents in relationships of different lengths and levels of committment are shown in Tables 1 and 2. Tables 3 and 4 show the frequency of reported child abuse and abuse witnessed between the respondents' parents. Table 1 Length of Respondents‘ Current or Most Recent Dating Relationship Length Proportion of Respondents Less than 1 month 11.6% 1 to 2 months 13.4% 3 to 6 months 20.4% 7 to 12 months 17.1% 1 to 2 years 22.7% 3 to 5 years 10.1% 6 or more years 1.2% Note. N-648 22 Table 2 Depth of Committment of Respondents' Current or Most Recent Dating Relationship Depth Proportion of Respondents Dating nonexclusively 28.8% Dating exclusively 44.4% Discussing marriage 17.6% Engaged 3.3% Living together 1.8% Note. N-653 Table 3 Frequency of Endorsement of Parent-Child Violence Items Item Percent Reporting Threaten to hit you or throw something at you 38.5 Throw something at you 15.8 Push, grab, or shove you 36.7 Spank you 78.1 Slap you 48.0 Kick, bite, or hit you 9.7 Beat you up 2.1 Threaten you with a gun or knife 0.9 Use a gun or knife on you 0.1 N016. N2647 23' Table 4 Frequency of Endorsement of Parent-Parent Violence Items Item Percent Reporting Threaten to hit or throw something at each other 23.0 Throw, hit,kick, or smash something 31.1 Throw something at each other 11.0 Push, grab, or shove each other ' 13.7 Slap each other 7.0 Kick, bite, or hit the other 4.9 Beat up the other 2.4 Threaten each other with a gun or knife 3.0 Use a gun or knife on each other 0.4 Note. N-646 mm Subjects were administered the CRAO. This instrument was designed to assess the frequency and intensity of both appropriate and coercive behaviors in the domains of conflict resolution, verbal/emotional interactions, and sexual interaction. Development of the CRAO was undertaken by Curtis, Maguin & Stollak (1987) to bring together and expand upon previous studies of marital violence which have used stnrctured clinical interviews (e.g., Walker, 1979) and/or brief questionnaires covering only selected and narrow areas of behavior (e.g., Straus, 1979; Hudson & McIntosh, 1981) to assess the level of violence and/or coercion between couples. The CRAO integrated these three areas of interaction into one asessament device utilizing a common methodological format. 2 4 The first section of the CRAO consisted often questions concaming demographic characteristics of the respondent. Another set of demographic questions on the CRAO included items that pertain to the marital, educational, and occupational status of the respondents' parents. The remainder of the first section included several scales designed to assess the dating and sexual histories of the respondent. In addition, information concaming characteristics of the current or most recent relationship was gathered. This included length of involvement, depth of commitment, and degree of sexual intimacy. Also, items asked about the frequency of alcohol and/or drug use in the context of the current or most recent relationship. The final section of the first part of the questionnaire included two scales to measure the degree of violence in the respondent's childhood. The items comprising these scales were taken from the CTS (Straus, 1979). The first sale asked the respondent to indicate which of nine violent behaviors he or she ever saw his/her parents engage in. The second scale asked the respondent which of nine violent behaviors his or her parents ever exhibited in their encounters with him or her. The first set of items in the main body of the questionnaire (nos. 1-67) asked the respondent to report on the frequency of a variety of conflict resolution behaviors employed between him/herself and his/her current or most recent boy/girlfriend. Each item is a behavior that could be employed to resolve interpersonal conflict, and the respondent was asked to rate approximately how many time he/she used this method of resolving conflict with his/her current or most recent boy/girlfriend by way of a frequency scale,1 to 5, where 1-never, 2. 2 to 4 times, 3- 5 to 10 times, 4-11 to 20 times, and 5. 21 or more times during the course of the relationship. Each subject was then asked to report on the methods used by his or her boy/girlfriend using the same rating scale. Items comprising this scale were taken from a number of different sources in an attempt to represent the widest possible range of conflict resolution behavior. Many items representative of violent behavior were taken from Straus' (1979) CTS. Items representing positive problem solving were derived 2 5 from Meichenbaum's (1977) problem solving steps. Other items were derived from clinical reports of battered women and their violent husbands. Some Items were the result of brainstorming possible conflict resolution behaviors, with the general categories of positive, avoiding, negative (nonviolent), and violent behaviors guiding development. A second set of items (nos. 68-93) measured the frequency of varying styles of emotionally and/or psychologically abusive behavior that might take place between dating couples. A third set of items (nos. 94-123) were designed to assess the frequency of various sexual behaviors between dating couples. The purpose of these items was to determine the frequency of mutually pleasurable and acceptable sexual behavior between dating couples as well as the frequency of coerced, unpleasant sexual behavior experienced and/or perpetrated by the respondents. The items from these two other sections of the CRAO were not utilized in the present study. These items are not posed in the context of conflict resolution. Rather, they measure whether or not any of these behaviors have ever occurred in the relationship, regardless of context. However, the first 67 items refer specifically to behaviors occurring during the resolution of a conflict situation. Hence, these first 67 items were the focus of the present study. Also included as part of the questionnaire was a 15 item validity scale, which is a modification of the Short-Form Marital Conventionalization (MC) Scale (Edmonds,1967; Edmonds, Withers, 8 Dibatista,1972). The original scale as developed by Edmonds (1967) was designed to be administered to married couples. Hence, the wording of some of the items was modified slightly so that the scale would be applicable to unmarried dating couples. The scale consists of 15 statements which the respondent marked either true or false as they applied to the relationship reported on. This validity scale was included as a check for random responding by subjects, as well as to explore the relationship between respondents portraying their relationship in the direction of social desirability and reports of physical, emotional and/or sexual abuse Maguin, Curtis, 8 Stollak, 1987). Questionnaires with extreme scores were not included ' 2 6 in the analysis. W. The length of the current or most recent relationship was measured on a seven-point scale, with one being the shortest length (less than a month) and seven being the longest (six or more years). This was the length of relationship scale. The level of commitment in the current or most recent relationship was measured on a six-point scale from dating nonexclusively to marriage (1 to 6). This was termed the depth of committment scale. History of child abuse was measured by the respondent indicating which of nine abusive behaviors he or she ever experienced at the hands of his/her parents. The items were taken from Straus' (1979) Conflict Tactics Scale. One of the nine items, “spanked“, was marked by approximately 80% of the respondents. It was felt that since this was appareme such a normative behavior In this sample that it should not be considered for the purposes of the study as an abusive behavior, and was thus not Included in the analysis. Each item was scored as zero or one, for not checked or checked. Each respondents' score on this scale was the resulting sum of the items. This scale was called the child violence scale. The violence between parents witnessed by the respondents was measured by a similar nine item scale taken from Straus (1979). This scale was termed the parental violence scale. Again, respondents were asked to indicate which of the behaviors they ever witnessed their parents engage in or display. A score was derived for this scale by summing the responses based on the zero-one scoring. A dating violence received scale was derived by scoring respondents for the level of violence received in their current or most recent relationship based on reports of 17 possible violent conflict resolution behaviors received from their partner. These 17 items were selected from the second half of the CRAO which has items that are identical to those in the first half, but where the question asked is which of these have you experienced as opposed to inflicted. These 17 items were taken from the moderate and severe violence factors 2 7 derived from the factor analysis. A recipient score was derived by summing the responses to these seventeen items. Thus, the lowest possible score is zero, and the maximum is 68. The coefficient alphas of each of the violence scales were calculated (T able 5) and were found to be of sufficient magnitude that the scales could be considered unidimensional measures of the reported events (Nunnally, 1978). Table 5 Coefficient Alphas of Subscales Scale Coefficient Alpha Violence Received in Relationship .906 Receiving Childhood Abuse .702 Witnessing Parental Violence .774 Note. N-630 mum. Respondents signed up for a psychology experiment entitled “Behavior in Dating Relationships.“ Each respondent was instructed on the sign-up sheet as to the date, time, and location of the ”experiment." Groups of approximately 40-50 undergraduates were recruited for each administration of the CRAO. These group administrations took place over a period of three months in large classrooms located on campus. Before each session, subjects were read a brief set of instructions describing the CRAO and how to fill it out. Subjects were told that the purpose of the study was to investigate conflict resolution behaviors of those involved in dating relationships. No reference was made to any specific behavior (e.g., violence) as one of interest. The CRAO took most respondents approximately 45 minutes to complete. Results The frequencies of endorsement for the first 67 items of the CRAO are presented in Table 17 In Appendix E. As expected, the typical, non-coercive conflict resolution behaviors were reported as rather common, wheras the more coercive and violent behaviors were reported less frequently. W. The first step in the formal analysis was a factor analysis of the conflict resolution behaviors endorsed (C RAO items 1-67). A principal components procedure was employed, with an oblique (oblimin) rotation of the initial factor solution. For the oblimin rotation, Delta was set at 0, creating a fairly oblique (correlated) solution (Nie, Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner 8 Bent, 1975). Seven factors were retained for final rotation. This decision was based on a SCREE test of the eigenvalues (see Figure 1, Appendix F) (Cattall, 1978). An oblique rotation was employed rather than an orthogonal rotation on the conceptual grounds that various conflict resolution behaviors are in fact related, and thus correlated. It is unreasonable to assume that an individual's use of one behavior (e.g., arguing) is not also sometimes accompanied by another different class or classes of behavior (e.g., yelling, hitting an object, or even before the yelling began, calm discussion). Further, an oblique rotation allows correlational relationships between factors (i.e., different classes of conflict resolution behaviors) to be examined. The seven factors derived included 59 of the scale's original 67 items. The item inclusion criterion was a factor loading of at least .30. In addition, three omitted Items, #4 “Used alcohol or drugs to settle the conflict”, #7 Prayed with him/her or alone“, and #45 ”Called police” were excluded for both conceptual and quantitative reasons. Conceptually, these items are not reflective of commonly employed conflict resolution strategies. They are more specialized or unusual strategies which were rarely endorsed. In fact, these non-violent items were endorsed less often than many of the violent items on 28 2 9 the questionnaire. Further, the exclusion of these three items significantly improved internal reliabilities (as calculated via coefficient alpha) of the factors initially derived from the factor analysis. Two items loaded on two of the factors. Item # 39, 'Soothed or calmed him/her“, loaded on the Appropriate/Generic factor as well as the Distraction factor. This is not surprising, as this conflict resolution behavior could refer to both a generally positive behavior and/or a way of directing attention away from conflict. Item if 47, 'Yelled or screamed at him/her” loaded on the Appropriate/General factor and the Verbal Attack/Coercion factor. This item loaded moderately on both factors, thus it was felt that it should be retained. This item clearly fits in the Verbal Attack/Coercion factor. However, it is not clear why it loaded on the Appropriate/Generic factor. Perhaps this Item was interpreted by some respondents as a normative, and not overly negative behavior. Thus, it is correlating with other, more clearly positive behaviors. With a listwise deletion of missing data (analysis performed using only cases with nonmissing values on the 64 items used), and the exclusion of subjects who indicated they were married, the final N for the factor analysis was 588. The items in each factor and their factor loadings are shown in Tables 18-24 in Appendix G. The first factor contains items that seem to reflect generally appropriate and/or common conflict resolution behaviors. Not all of the items are positive or facilitative. However, none of the items is violent, and none of them involves behavior that is overtly coercive. The second factor consists of items that are violent. These are all items that involve the use of some type of physically coercive behavior that is utilized to force the will of the actor upon the receiver in the resolution of a conflict. However, these are the moderate violence items on the CRAO. The items comprising the third factor generally reflect behaviors employed to avoid dealing directly with conflict. Factor four consists of the most violent items on the questionnaire. The fifth factor contains items which reflect attempts to avoid conflict by distracting the other person. Factor six represents generally negative verbal attack behaviors, that are not physically coercive. 3 0 These behaviors perhaps serve to amplify conflict, and are reflective of inappropriate, non-constructive conflict resolution behaviors. The seventh and final factor extracted is one which contains the items of violence directed at objects during conflict. Composite scale scores were computed based on the sum of the responses to the items In the seven factors using the 0-4 frequency scale of the CRAO. The internal consitency (coefficient alpha) of the items of each composite are presented in Table 6. The coefficient alphas are of sufficient magnitude such that each of the composite scales for the extracted factors appears to meet the conventional standards for satisfactory internal consistency (Nunnally, 1978). Table 6 Alpha Coefficients for CRAO Factors Factor Coefficient Alpha Appropriate/Generic .937 Moderate Violence .886 Avoid .854 Severe Wolenca .674 Distraction .842 Verbal Attack/Coercion .936 Symbolic/Indirect .725 Note. N-588 The Pearson interfactor correlations were calculated for the raw subscale scores. These are presented in Table 7. As can be seen, these correlations were for the most part moderate to high, indicating a great deal of interrelatedness among the subscales. The only two nonsignificant correlations 3 1 were between the Severe violence subscale and the Appropriate/Generic and Distract subscales. The Severe and Moderate violence scales were moderately correlated, while there was a much higher correlation between the Moderate and Symbolic violence subscales. There was a somewhat low correlation between the Severe violence subscale and the Symbolic violence subscales. The Verbal Attack/Coercion, Appropriate, and Avoid subscales were also highly correlated. The lowest significant correlations were between the Severe violence subscale and the Verbal Attack, Symbolic violence and Avoid subscales. There was also a low but significant correlation between the Moderate violence subscale and the Distract subscale. Otherwise, the factors generally appear to be moderately to highly intercorrelated. Table 7 Pearson Correlations Between Factors of the CRAO Factor 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 AppropGen Mod.Viol. Avoid SevereVioI. Distract Verbl.Attack Symblc. 1 1 .00 2 .35‘" 1 .00 3 .57‘“ .33“‘ 1 .00 4 .03 .27'“ .1 4*“ 1 .00 5 .64‘“ .21 ”' .38'“ -.01 1 .00 6 .75'" .75'“ .65’” .1 4*“ .49'” 1 .00 7 .54‘” .54‘” .52’” .1 7"" .32’" .63”* 1 .00 Notes. Correlations based on N-591 ‘“ p< .001 W. The second step of the analysis was to develop profiles of respondents through a cluster analysis of cases based on the scores on the composite subscales. These scores formed the data set for the cluster analysis. A k-means clustering technique (Dixon, 1981) was employed whereby the 3 2 cases were partitioned into clusters based on these raw score-based composite scales. For clustering, the data were standardized univariately using the standard deviation of the complete sample. The final number of cases utilized for the cluster analysis (after deletions due to missing data) was 635. After experimentation with solutions of varying numbers of clusters, a five cluster solution was viewed as the most parsimonious without losing important detail, given the number of subjects. Cluster populations, means, and standard deviations are presented in Tables 8-10. In general, these clusters seemed to represent distinctive and logical conceptual groupings of cases. The first cluster contained only three cases (this cluster remained even in a four cluster solution). This cluster clearly included the most violent of the respondents, with scores on the Severe and Moderate violence subscales elevated well above the mean. These respondents were deemed Life Threateners. However, it is important to note that any conclusions concaming these respondents should be tempered by the large standard deviations of the subscale means. For this reason, and because of the small number of respondents in the cluster, it was decided to exclude this cluster from any subsequent analyses. The second cluster included those cases that are close to themean on all the subscales except for Severe violence, on which they were below the mean (in fact, they have the lowest mean on this composite). They were slightly below the mean on the moderate violence subscale, but were still within one standard deviation. These respondents were termed Positive/Average Conflict Confronters. The third cluster seemed to be comprised of those respondents who were below the mean on all seven subscales. This cluster may represent a group of people who generally respond passively to conflict, or perhaps are in short term relationships and thus have not experienced a great deal of conflict in the relationship. This group of people might best be called lnexperienced Nonresponders. The fourth cluster consisted of the fewest people aside from cluster one. These respondents reported a great deal of violent behavior, but did not report perpetrating the most severe acts of violence. They were well 3 3 above the mean on the Moderate violence scale, and are elevated on the Severe violence scale, although not by one standard deviation or more. In addition, they were very high on all of the other scales. This group was termed the Coercive/Active Conflict Resolvers. The fifth cluster contains people who were above the mean on all of the scales except the two violence scales, on which they fell near the mean. These respondents were called the Belligerent/Active Conflict Resolvers. Table 8 Cluster Populations Cluster N Life Threateners 3 Positive/Average 255 lnexperienced 203 Coercive/Active 39 Belligerent/Active 135 Total 635 Table 9 34 Cluster Means on Seven CRAO Subscales Approp' Moderae Severe Verbd Syn'bolic Ctrster Genera Violence Avoid Violence Distract Attack Violence Lie Threateners 29.67 12.67 23.67 7.33 1 1.00 31 .33 6.00 Positive/Average 31.05 1 .08 10.17 0.03 14.40 13.40 1.20 lnexperienced 16.70 0.19 5.73 0.03 7.53 5.40 0.39 Coercive/Active 46.08 13.84 18.26 0.18 16.26 35.44 6.38 Belligerent/Active 42.89 2.08 17.78 0.03 16.40 27.43 3.72 Grand Mean 29.66 1.81 10.94 0.08 12.72 15.18 1.82 Table 10 Cluster Standard Deviations on Seven CRAO Factors Amopi Modeiae Severe Verbd Synbolic Chalet General Violence Avoid Violence Distract Attack Violence Lle Threateners 5.51 16.74 1 1 .02 1 .53 2.65 15.57 4.58 Positive/Average 7.94 1 .78 4.55 0.27 3.82 6.28 1 .30 lnexperienced 7.25 0.57 3.41 0.30 3.57 4.26 0.79 Coercive/Active 6.07 6.32 6.24 0.51 3.90 9.19 2.83 Belligerent/Active 7.28 2.1 1 5.77 0.28 3.48 8.77 2.12 35 W. The five dependent variables were subjected to a 4 (group) x 2 (sex) multivariate analysis of variance. The multivariate F for group was significant (p<.001). However, the multivariate F of the interaction of group x sex was not significant (p>.10). Since there was no multivariate effect for the interaction of group and sex, univariate F's were examined with males and females combined for each of the dependent variables by groups. W. The relationships between each of the five hypothesized predictors of coercive behavior In dating relationships and the groups resulting from the cluster analysis were tested by one-way ANOVAs, with group membership as the independent variable. The length of the current relationship was a significant predictor of group membership. The mean of the lnexperienced group (group three) was significantly lower (p<.05) than the means of all other groups according to a Tukey HSD procedure. Also, the means of the Coercive/Active and the Belligerent/Active groups (groups four and five) were significantly higher than the mean of the Positive/Average group (group two) (p<.05) (see Table 11). There was no difference between the lnexperienced and Positive/Average groups, as well as no difference between the Belligerent/Active and Coercive/Active groups. 36 Table 11 Means and F Ratios of Dependent Variables For Each Group Varlmle Pcsiive/ lnexperienced Coercive/Active Beigerert/ F(3,619) Mace m Length of Relationship 3.783 2.73), 4.596 423c 43.42“- Depth of Relationship 2.063 1.76:, 2.41 a 2,19a 1050‘m Child “demo 1 .47 1 .44 1 .97 1 .82 2.69' Parent W 0.92a 0.77a.b 1.58c 1 .27c 4.65“ Receiving Violence In Curran “Mbmmp 1.368 0.373 1 1 .44b.c 2'89b,d 80.20." Note. Within each row, means with different subscripts differ at the .05 level of significance according to a Tukey HSD procedure. Depth of involvement in the current or most recent relationship was also a significant predictor of group membership overall. The means of the Coercive/Active, Positive/ Average, and Belligerent/Active groups were higher (more deeply committed) than the mean of the lnexperienced group at the .05 level of significance. Experiencing of abuse by parents was a significant predictor of group membership overall, although there were no significant between group differences. Also, witnessing violence between parents was a significant 3 7 predictor. According to the Tukey procedure, the means of the Belligerent/Active and Coercive/Active groups were significantly higher (more parental violence witnessed) (p<.05) than the mean of the lnexperienced group. Finally, being the recipient of abusive behavior in the dating relationship was the strongest predictor of group membership. The mean of the Coercive/Active group was significantly higher at the .05 level than all other groups. Also, the mean of the Belligerent/Active group was significantly higher than the means of the lnexperienced and Positive/Average groups at the .05 lavel. W. In the overall ANOVA, the respondents in the lnexperienced cluster had shorter relationships than respondents in all other clusters. The respondents in the Coercive/Active and Belligerent/Active clusters had longer duration relationships than respondents in the Positive/Average group, although length of relationship did not differ between the Coercive/Active and Belligerent/Active groups. Respondents in the lnexperienced cluster also reported relationships that were less committed than respondents in all other groups. These respondents in the Belligerent/Active and Coercive/Active clusters reported witnessing more parent violence than those in the lnexperienced cluster. Finally, those in the Coercive/Active cluster reported receiving more violence in the current or most recent relationship than those in all other clusters. Also, those in the Belligerent/Active cluster reported more violence in the current or most recent relationship than those in the Positive/Average and the lnexperienced clusters. W. Given that the variables selected as hypothesized predictors of group membership were significant predictors of group membership, a discriminant function analysis was employed to examine which variables had the most power to discriminate between the four groups. As with the oncway analysis of variance, since the group x sex interaction in the multivariate analysis of variance was not significant, an overall analysis of 38 males and females combined was performed. Of a maximum of three possible functions (in discriminant function analysis, maximum number of functions-ngroups-1), two were significant, with a combined x2 (15, N3592)-322.18, p<.001. After removal of the first function, x2 (8, N-592) was 68.14 (p<.001). However, after removal of the second function, x2 (3, N-592) was only 2.13, p>.50. Table 12 Characteristics of Discriminant Functions Derived Function I Function lI Function Ill Eigenvalue .542 .1 19 .004 Canonical Correlation .593 .326 .060 Percent of Variance 81.53% 17.92% 0.55% Wilks Lambda (After Removal) .577 .890 .996 Notes. N-592 Number of independent variables-5 Number of groups-4 The canonical correlations (RC) presented in Table 12 are correlations between the entire set of predictors and the criteria of group classifications. The eigenvalues (related to the canonical correlations by the formula 91 =- chi) and their associated canonical correlations taken together reflect the relative ability of each function to separate the groups. As can be seen, the canonical 39 correlations of the first two functions are moderately high, while the correlation for function three is negligible, thus confirming the significance of the first two functions and the relative nonlmportance of the third. Consequently, only the first two functions (I and II) were employed for the remainder of the discriminant analysis computations. Table 13 presents the mean discriminant scores for each group on the respective functions. The group centroids summarize the group locations in the space defined by the discriminant functions. It appears that the first discriminant function is discriminanting the Coercive/Active group from the other three. The second discriminant function separates the Coercive/Active group from the BelligerentlActive group and the Positive/Average group, but the discriminating ability of this function Is somewhat less than that of the first function. Overall, the dscnmination of the three nonviolent groups from each other is not as clear as that of the violent group from these three. Table 13 Mean Discriminant Functions Evaluated at Group Means (Group Centroids) Cluster Function 1 Function 2 Positive/Average -0.092 0.207 lnexperienced -0.654 -0.351 Coercive/Active 2.322 -0.706 BelligerentlActive 0.481 0.371 Note. N-592 Figure 2 (see Appendix H), a plot of the four group centroids on the two discriminant functions, illustrates the separation of the four groups by the 40 dscriminant functions. This again shows that the first function serves primarily to distinguish between the Coercive/Active group and the lnexperienced group. The other two groups fall in the middle, and are not as clearly distinguished, although the first function does discriminate them somewhat from the Coercive/Active group. The second function does not serve to cfiscriminate among the four groups, although it slightly discriminates the Coercive/Active group form the BelligerentlActive and Positive/Average groups. However, these-two groups (BelligerentlActive and Positive/Average) are very close together on the second function. As can be seen geometrically, the magnitude of discrimination of the second function is not as great as the discriminanting ability of the first function and it seems that function one is the primary discriminator. WW Table 14 presents the standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients, which represent the relative contributions of the variables to the two functions. Receiving violence in a relationship seems to make the greatest contribution to the first function. All of the other variables appear to be of minor importance on this function, with the exception of length, which makes a moderate contribution. Oh function two, receiving violence has a high negative coefficient. Length of relationship makes the greatest positive contribution to the second function. The other discriminating variables appear to make negligible contributions to this function. 41 Table 14 Standardized Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients Variable Function 1 Function 2 Receiving violence in current relationship 0.813 -0.582 Receiving parental violence 0.059 0.072 Witnessing parental violence 0.191 0.103 Length of current or most recent relationship 0.466 0.869 Depth of current or most recent relationship 0.000 0.006 The relationship between the individual discriminating variables and the functions can be seen in Table 15, which presents the Pearson product-moment correlations between the functions and the five discriminating variables. According to Klecka (1980), the vector of correlations of variables with a discriminant function, and not the vector of the partial discriminant functions (as in Table 14), provides the most descriptive summary of the behavioral correlates of the discriminant function. 42 Table 15 Correlations Between Discriminant Functions and Discriminating Variables Variable Function 1 Function 2 Receiving violence in current relationship 0.863‘ 0.489 Witnessing parental violence 0.1 96* 0.1 04 Receiving parental violence 0.144' 0.058 Length of current or most recent relationship 0.541 0.804’ Depth of current or most recent relationship 0.291 0.319’ Note. Correlations marked with an asterisk in the first column correlate more highly with Function 1 than with Function 2; the reverse is true in the second column. It is evident from these correlations that receiving violence in the current relationship was most strongly related to function one. Since this function served to discriminate the most violent group from the least violent group, the results suggest that receiving violence in the relationship is associated with perpetrating violence In the relationship, and is a useful variable in discriminating violent from nonviolent groups. Further, this variable was moderately and negatively correlated with the second discriminant function, indicating that not receiving violence in the current relationship was associated with this function. Since the second function most clearly discriminated 43 between the Coercive/Active group and the Positive/Average and Belfigerent/Active groups, this would seem to indicate that these latter two groups were those who report not receiving violence. However, in referring back to the ANOVA, it is evident that this was not the case. Generally, it is unclear how well function two discriminantes among any of the groups, and interpretations made on the basis of group locations on this function must obviously be made with some reservation. Having witnessed parental violence during childhood and having been the victim of abuse during childhood were also more highly correlated with the first function, although these correlations were low, and not much higher than the correlations of these variables with the second function. Thus, these two history of violence variables were somewhat associated with the Coercive/Active group, but this relationship was not strong. Length of relationship was highly correlated with the second function, but also had a strong correlation with function one as well. This may suggest that those in the longest term relationships are not in the most violent group, while being in a relationship of moderate duration is associated with violence (i.e., the moderate correlation with function one). The correlations of depth of involvement and the two discriminant functions, while lower than those betweeen the functions and length of relationship, indicate the same pattern, with highly committed relationships being associated with the second function, and relationships characterized by moderate levels of committment correlated with the first function. However, these differences in correlations are not as pronounced as those involving length of relationship. W. As shown in Table 16, together, the two functions correctly classified 44.7% of the most violent subjects (Coercive/Active cluster), 68.9% of the Positive/Average subjects, 59.8% of the lnexperienced subjects, and only 9.6% of the BelligerentlActive cluster of subjects. The two discriminant ' functions correctly classified 51.9% of all cases correctly, which represents reasonable improvement over chance classification (25%) of subjects into the 44 four groups. The discriminant functions incorrectly classified most of the subjects from the BelligerentlActive group, erroneously predicting them as members of the Positive/Average group. Also, the two functions classified less than half of the subjects in the most violent group correctly. However, this is not surprising when consideration is made of the inabilty of the second function to discriminate these two groups at all, and the minor discrimination of these two groups by the first function. The discriminant functions more accurately classified the subjects in the two nonviolent groups. Table 16 Discriminant Function Classification Results Predicted Cluster Membership Actual Cluster No. of Cases PosJAv. Inexp. C/A BIA Positive/Average 235 68.9% 26.8% 2.1% 6.4% 162 63 5 8 lnexperienced 194 39.9% 59.8% 0.0% 0.5% 77 1 16 0 1 Coercive/Active 38 28.9% 7.9% 44.7% 1 8.4% 1 1 3 17 7 BelligerentlActive 125 67.2% 16.8% 6.4% 9.6% 84 21 8 1 2 Note. Numbers under percentages are actual number of cases classified. Discussion Factor analyses of previous instruments measuring conflict resolution behavior have yielded either separate physical and nonphysical abuse factors (Hudson 8 McIntosh, 1981) or factors distinguishing between reasoning, verbal aggression, and physical violence (Straus, 1979). These categories seemed to represent a rather limited view of the great variety of possible conflict resolution behaviors. The administration of the CRAO in this study revealed a wealth of information concaming conflict resolution behavior in college students' dating relationships. Specifically, it revealed that college students utilize a wider variety of conflict resolution behaviors than simply physical violence, verbal aggression, and reasoning in their dating relationships. The factor analysis of the CRAO in the present study yielded seven correlated factors, each representing distinct categories of conflict resolution behavior. The first factor contains items that represent generally appropriate conflict resolution behaviors. These include behaviors that represent positive problem solving strategies (e.g., suggesting possible solutions, acknowledging feelings about the problem, etc.) as well as more generic sorts of conflict resolution behavior (e.g., arguing or becoming angry). Even though some of the behaviors represented by this factor do not fall under the rubric of positive problem solving (e.g., as discussed by Meichenbaum (1977)), none of the items represent behavior that is physically coercive (i.e., an attempt to impose a problem solution by the use of physical force or threat thereof). This factor is similar to Straus' Reasoning Scale, but contains more items that represent a broader range of positive problem solving behavior beyond just reasoning. The second nonviolent factor, the Avoidance factor, contains, as the name implies, items which represent behaviors that are an attempt to avoid the conflict. Ignoring the problem, changing the topic of discussion, or leaving the room are clearly ways of avoiding a conflict situation. This class of conflict 45 4 6 resolution behaviors was not included by Straus (1979) on the CTS or Hudson 8 McIntosh (1981) on the ISA, thus not taking into account the posilble relationship between avoiding a conflict and using violence or any other behavior during a conflict. The items that comprised the Distraction factor were similar to those of the Avoid factor, and certainly represented behaviors that could be utilized to avoid conflict. However, they were different in that, for the most part, they represented attempts to distract the other person by doing something that would divert attention away from the conflict. This distracting behavior is generally something which is nonaggressive, moving toward the other type of behavior (e.g., becoming affectionate or making sexual advances). However, one of the items, ”Went for walk, jogged or worked out“ did not seem to clearly fit in this factor, and would seem to more appropriately go into the Avoid factor. However, this item did not load on that factor. Respondents may not have viewed this item as a way of directly avoiding conflict, but rather as a more positive behavior designed to placate or distract the other person or possibly the actor himself/herself. The Verbal Attack/Coercion factor of the CRAO was made up of items representative of behaviors that are verbally coercive, such as talking down to the person, lecturing the person, and nagging. Although not physically coercive, these behaviors are attempts to make the other person submit to the will of the actor in a conflict by belittling and/or verbal badgering, and represent what would be deemed verbal coercion The Symbolic/Indirect Violence factor was the first of the three violence factors of the CRAO. Although it only comprised four items of moderate to low loadings, it appeared to be comprised of items that were distinctive enough to warrant this being a factor unto itself. These items clearly represent violent behavior that is directed at objects rather than a person. It is possible that in some instances kicking an object or throwing an object may be an attempt to frighten, shock, or intimidate the person into submitting in a conflict, or performed out of the fnrstration aroused by the continuing failure to resolve the 4 7 conflict. These two factors (Verbal Attack/Coercion and Symbolic Violence) roughly parallel Straus' (1979) Verbal Aggression scale of the CTS. However, it is clear from the present results that there were at least two unique domains of behavior represented by these two factors, and not one verbal aggression factor as Straus proposes. They also were similar to Hudson 8 McIntosh's (1981) nonphysical abuse factor of their ISA Again, however, it is felt that the two separate factors derived from the CRAO explain this dimension of conflict resolution in greater detail. Verbal aggression (e.g., yelling at a person) and kicking a chair are two very different behaviors that could very well result in different conseuencas for those involved. The Moderate Violence factor of the CRAO contained items that are representative of moderately violent conflict resolution behaviors. These are behaviors that might be employed to physically force the actor's will on another during conflict. These behaviors could result in accidental or minor injury to the recipient, although one of them, 'Choked or strangles him/her" seems to be more serious than the the other items, since this behavior could result in death. However, most of the behaviors represented by the items in this factor would generally not be life threatening. The Severe Violence factor included items that represent serious, life threatening violence. These include the use of a weapon (e.g., a knife or gun), or other behaviors (e.g., strangulation) that would clearly endanger the life of the recipient. These two violence factors, moderate violence and severe violence, roughly correspond to Straus' violence and life-threatening violence scales from the CTS. They also are similar to the physical abuse scale of the ISA. The factor analysis performed by Straus (1979) utilized an orthogonal rotation, in which the factors are assumed to be independent and unrelated. Thus, he does not present interfactor correlations. Hudson 8 McIntosh (1981) also report an orthogonal rotation of the factors form the ISA. The present factor analysis of the CRAO utilized an oblique rotation, thus allowing for correlations 4 8 between factors. The correlations obtained indicated that all of the factors were for the most part moderately correlated at a high level of statistical significance. Specifically, these correlations suggest that those respondents who reported utilizing appropriate conflict resolution behaviors also utilize verbal attack behaviors characterized by items in that factor, as well as using dstracting, symbolically violent, and avoidance behaviors. The Positive/Appropriate factor was not related to the use of severe violence, and only moderately with the use of moderate violence. The use of verbal attack, symbolic violence, and moderate violence are also moderately intercorrelated. However, Avoid is also moderately correlated with Symbolic and Verbal Attack, but not with Moderate violence. Finally, Severe violence is moderately correlated with Moderate violence, as might be expected, but has a low to nonsignificant correlation with the other factors. The overall pattern of moderate intercorrelations seemed to suggest that respondents are reporting the use of widely diverse conflict resolution behaviors, and generally tend not to utilize one or a few classes of behavior(s) to the exclusion of others. However, factors that defined behaviors that are the most divergent from each other, such as ApprOpriate/Generic and Severe violence are not correlated. Further, factors that are not as different, but which do not represent conceptually similar classes of behavior, for example, Severe violence and Verbal Attack, are correlated, but only at a modest level. Thus, those factors which represent similar, nonviolent forms of conflict resolution behavior are most highly correlated. However, It is interesting to note that the highest interfactor correlation is between the Appropriate/Generic and the Verbal Attack factors. Perhaps the behaviors represented by the items comprising these two factors are considered normative modes of conflict resolution, and respondents do not characterize the Verbal Attack items as 'bad", or different from what have been labelled the appropriate, positive behaviors. After the derivation of the seven factors from the CRAO items, the next step in the analysis was to cluster respondents according to their scores on 4 9 composite subscales derived from each of the factors. This procedure yielded five clusters, or types of respondents. A typology of conflict resolution was thus developed based upon these seven groups of conflict resolution behavior. This particular type of analysis has not previously been reported in either the spouse abuse or dating violence literatures. The Ufa Threatener cluster consisted of only three subjects. These were respondents who reported a great deal of life threatening violence. These subjects had very high responses on the two violence factors, as well as somewhat elevated scores on the Avoid and Symbolic violence subscales. Otherwise, respondents in this cluster had scores on all other subscales that were generally close to the mean. They obtained a very high score on the Severe Wolence subscales, but their score on the Moderate Violence factor was less than one standard deviation above the mean of that factor (however, the standard deviation of that subscale for this group was actually greater than the mean on the subscale for this group). Thus it seemed that these respondents, while using some moderately violent and nonviolent coercive strategies were primarily using a great deal of life threatening violence. It is unclear whether these respondents were responding In a way to mislead the investigator (a check of the validity scale of these protocols did not indicate random responding, nor did an examination of the pattern of reponses to the CRAO conflict behavior items), or if they were accurately reporting a great deal of violence in their current or most recent relationship. There was some doubt about the validity of these respondents' protocols, since these respondents were reporting implausibly high levels of violence in their dating relationships (e.g., reporting having shot their partner with a gun four to ten times). Due to these doubts about the validity of these protocols the small number of respondents in this cluster, these respondents were excluded from further analyses. However, it is important to note that these very violent subjects may have been honestly reporting their behavior, as other studies in dating violence (e.g., Sigelman et al, 1984) have consistently noted a small group of such highly coercive respondents. 5 O The second cluster, which consisted of 40% (N-255) of the respondents, was designated the Positive/Average cluster. Respondents in this cluster were near the mean on all of the subscales. Also, they had the lowest score (along with the lnexperienced group) on the severe violence factor, and the second lowest on the moderate violence factor. The proportion of males and females In this cluster was similar to the proportion in the entire sample. Respondents in this cluster report receiving very little violence in the current relationship, current or most recent relationships of average length and depth, and average amounts (low) of childhood violence. The discriminant function analysis was only able to clearly ciscriminate this cluster from the most coercive cluster. This cluster, the most populous, is felt to represent the typical respondent, who is using an average amount of a wide range of conflict resolution strategies, but who does not typically exhibit violence in a conflict. The third cluster, the second largest, contains 32% (N-203) of the respondents. It also has nearly equal proportions of males and females. This group of respondents were designated the lnexperienced conflict resolvers. Across all subscales they have scores on the predictors that are well below the mean. This group of respondents reported the shortest and least committed relationships. Thus, the low scores are most likely not indicative of passivity in conflict (although in some individual cases this may be true), but rather reflect the low number and intensity of conflicts experienced in a short, uncommitted relationship. They also report receiving the least amount of violence in the current or most recent relationship, which seems to further support the notion of a low number and intensity of conflicts. The fourth cluster, which contained 6% (N-39) of the respondents, was named the Coercive/Active cluster. This was the smallest of the four clusters retained for analysis. These respondents were above the mean on all the subscales. They had the highest scores on the Appropriate, Verbal Attack/Coercive, and Symbolic/Indirect Violence factors, as well as the highest score (by several standard deviations) on the Moderate Violence subscale. 5 1 Also, of the four clusters retained for analysis, this cluster had the highest score on the Severe Violence subscale. In general, these respondents seemed to be utilizing every type of conflict resolution behavior with great frequency. Of particular note, they were using a great deal of negative behaviors and moderately violent behaviors but were also using many appropriate strategies, and have not resorted to a great deal of severe as those in the Life Threatening cluster. This cluster consisted of 26 females and 14 males from the sample. This dsproportionately high number of females in the Coercive/Active cluster was an unexpected finding, to say the least. Other studies of dating violence (e.g., Laner 8 Thompson, 1982; Sigelman et al, 1984) have indicated that approximately equal numbers of males and females perpetrate violent acts in dating relationships. The high level of coercion reported by these women may be acts of self-defense or in retaliation against male perpetrated coercion. Matthews (1984) found that when violent acts occurred in a dating relationship, 39% of the males and 61% of the females indicated that they fought back in response. This data Indicates that there may be some validity to the idea that when females are assaulted, they tend to fight back the majority of the time. However, the present data does not allow a determination of a causal sequence, and one could just as easily say that these respondents receive coercion because they perpetrate it first. The clinical case literature on spouse abuse Indicates that in the majority of cases where women use violence it is in self-defense or in retaliation against male initiated assaults (Walker, 1979; 1984). However, much of this literature is based on abused women reporting on their husbands In therapy. Past findings in the area of dating violence have clearly demonstrated that the perpetraticn of violent acts in dating relationships is reciprocal (Sigelman at al, 1984; Lane 8 Gwartney-Gibbs, 1985). In the present study this notion is further supported, with receiving violence in the current relationship being the most powerful predictor of perpetrating violence. However, the factors that might contribute to a dating couple becoming mutually coercive are unclear due to the limitations inherent in survey research. 5 2 The data in the present study indicated that respondents in this cluster reported being in somewhat longer and more committed relationships than those in other clusters. This finding is consistent with findings by Laner 8 Thompson (1982) and Sigelman et al (1984) where subjects in longer and more serious relationships reported more violence in the relationship. Being in a more serious and ongoing relationship may bring more long standing, serious conflict which could potetially lead to more extreme forms of conflict resolution behaviors. Also, given the deeper level of commitment, there may be a greater pull to maintain the relationship regardless of the amount of conflict and resultant coercion. Respondents in the Coercive/Active cluster reported having witnessed more violence between parents than those in the two nonviolent clusters, althogh there is no statistically significant difference in the amount of abuse received as a child, although the mean for this group is higher than the means of the other three on this predictor. A general association between violence in the dating relationship and experiencing violence in the family of origin is consistent with findings in almost all studies testing this hypothesis in both the spouse abuse literature (e.g., Straus at al, 1980) and the dating violence literature (e.g., Laner 8 Thompson, 1982; Bernard 8 Bernard, 1983). The discriminant functions distinguished this cluster most clearly from the other clusters. However, the discriminant functions correctly classified only 45% of the respondents into the Coercive/Active cluster, while incorrectly classifying 29% of the respondents in this cluster into the Positive/Average cluster, and 18% of the respondents into the BelligerentlActive cluster. This may be indicating that some of the respondents in this cluster are not as coercive, and based on the discriminating variables they cannot be accurately classified into this cluster and are placed into a less coercive or noncoercive cluster. In general, the respondents in the Coercive/Active cluster reported utilizing a great number of varying conflict resolution strategies. This may simply be the result of a higher level of conflict in their dating relationships. Unfortunately, it was beyond the scope of the present study to assess the level of conflict in the 5 3 respondents' current or most recent relationship. The fifth cluster was designated the BelligerentlActive cluster. Respondents in this cluster were well above the mean on all factors, except the violence factors, on which they were near the mean. On the nonviolent factors, they were very similar to the Coercive/Active group, except they scored slightly lower on the Verbal Attack subscale than that group. This cluster consisted of 135 respondents (21% of the respondents), with nearly equal numbers of females and males. The respondents In this cluster reported receiving more violence than members of the Positive/Average and lnexperienced clusters. Respondents in this cluster reported slightly longer duration and slightly more committed relationships than those respondents in the Positive/Average and lnexperienced clusters as well. They generally did not report receiving more violence in childhood, and although not statistically significant, they reported witnesssing more violence in childhood. Thus, repondents in this cluster seem to represent individuals who utilize many strategies to resolve conflict, as with the Coercive/Active cluster, but not as many violent or negative strategies. The cfiscriminant functions were not able to clearly distinguish this group from the Coercive/Active cluster, and placed it somewhere between this group and the other two noncoercive groups. Notably, the discriminant functions were able to correctly classify only 10% of the respondents into this cluster. Sixty seven percent of the respondents in this cluster were incorrectly classified into the Positive/Average cluster, and 17% were misclassified into the lnexperienced cluster. It seems that the chosen discriminating variables were not able to distinguish these respondents from those in the Positive/Average cluster. It may be that these variables were good at distinguishing extremes (i.e., coercive from appropriate), but they may not be able to distinguish this noncoercive, yet negative and aggressive group. The discriminant function analysis attempted to utilize a set of variables to distinguish between four of the clusters derived. The first discriminant function was able to distinguish the Coercive/Active cluster from the other three. The 54 second discriminant function generally did not serve to clearly discriminate the groups, although it did discriminate slightly the Coercive/Active group from the BelligerentlActive and Positive/Average groups. Receiving violence in the current or most recent relationship was most highly correlated with function one. This correlation with the first function is consistent with this variable as a very strong predictor of coercion as demonstrated by the ANOVA, since function one is most clearly associated with the Coercive/Active group. Length of relationship and depth of relationship were correlated with the first function, but these two variables had their highest correlation with the second function. This, along with the ANOVA for length and depth may suggest that the relationship between length and depth and coercion is not linear, but rather an inverted 'U' relationship, where those in the shortest, least committed and longest, most committed relationships do not report as much violence as those in relationships of moderate length and depth. Violent dating relationships may continue to a point, but with high levels of conflict and violence may not last as long as relationships without violence. Thus,those relationships of greatest length and duration are generally not violent. Witnessing parental violence and receiving violence as a child were more highly correlated with the first function than with the second. However, these correlations are low, and do not differ greatly from the correlations with the second function. However, the relationship between childhood violence and the four groups as yielded by the discriminant analysis is somewhat ambiguous at best. Sigelman et al (1984), in their discriminant analysis of abusive and nonabusive college students, found that being abused as a child discriminated violent females from nonviolent ones, but did not discriminate males. Their results were also somewhat weak as in the present study. Also, Stats 8 Pirog-Good (1987) did not find strong support for the idea that violence is intergenerational. However, in the present study these two variables were predictive overall of group status according the the ANOVA, but did have the least predictive power. It seems that history of childhood violence is somewhat 5 5 predictive of coercion in dating relationships. However, this historical variable clearly does not have the predictive power of variables associated with the current relationship. It may be that events in childhood simply may not have as great an effect on current behavior, and there may be more variability in the effect of these more temporally distal variables. Thus, being abused as a child certainly makes one more likely to be an abusive dating partner, but as Straus at al (1980) report, nearly half of the abusive respondents in their sample had not experienced childhood violence. Overall, the discriminant function analysis most strongly indicated that receiving violence in the current or most recent relationship is the most highly dscriminating variable of the four clusters. This seems to indicate, along with the highly significant ANOVA results concaming this variable, that coercion in dating relationships is reciprocal. When one partner is violent, the other tends to strike back Researchers in marital violence (e.g., Steinmetz, 1980; Straus, et al. 1980) have found clear indications that violence in marriages is reciprocal, as have investigators in the dating violence field (e.g., Sigelman, at al., 1984; Lane 8 Gwartney-Glbbs, 1985). Although these studies, as well as the present study, demonstrate the mutuality of such violence, there has been little but speculation concaming the development and maintainence of mutually coercive behaviors in the context of intimate, loving relationships. Due to the limitations of the survey method - the predominant research methodology (present study included) - little is known concaming the development and dynamics of violent behavior between intimates. Initial investigations in the field of marital violence, primarily by feminist oriented writers, tended to blame the male dominated social and political hierarchy for male violence against women (e.g., Dobash 8 Dobash, 1979). However, it is now apparent that violence is a mutual event, albeit some of the violence perpetrated by women Is certainly in self-defense against male initiated acts (Straus et al, 1980). Further, studies of dating violence to date have primarily gathered demographic data as predictors of violence, and have not examined more 5 6 psychological and/or personality variables that may be related to conflict resolution in dating relationships. Stats 8 Pirog-Good (1987) found that the personality dimensions of instrumentality and expressiveness influence receiving violence in dating relationships. However, these two dimensions did not influence the use of violence in relationships. Future studies should examine other personality characteristics of individuals in dating relationships that may predispose them to both use and receive violence in resolving conflict. Concepts such as mood, agoocontrol and resiliency, internal versus external locus of control, and others need to be investigated as predictors of dating violence so that more focused theoretical formulations concaming violence and conflict resolution in general may be made. Studies, to date, have only begun to provide the necessary information to understand the specific causal events within a relationship. But we have even less knowledge of events within the violent encounter itself that lead to violent behavior. Clearly, violent behavior within a relationship is a symptom of a dysfunctional system, where the man's experiencing might be similar to the woman's (although his behavior might often be the more dangerous of the two). Thus, couples, together, need to be a target of study, along with continued surveys of individuals. Further, it is believed that the continued study of dating couples rather than married couples would be beneficial. Dating couples are just beginning to form relationships, and early patterns that may lead to a lifetime of abuse, which might be obscured in couples who have been together a long time, may be uncovered. The present study determined that there are several different types of behaviors utilized to resolve conflict, and that these behaviors have more detailed texture than simply violent versus nonviolent. A more intensive study of a smaller number of couples, perhaps selected on the basis of a mass screening with an instrument such as the CRAO, is essential. Also, the data from the present study clearly Indicated that coercive conflict resolution behavior is reciprocal, and in large part dependent on the amount of time the couple has been together. 5 7 In future research, young dating couples (ideally high school or college couples) who have been together for varying lengths of time should be interviewed concaming experiences in their families of origin, the history of their dating relationships, and about the functioning of their current relationship. One historical variable that may be of particular interest in the context of conflict resolution behavior would be that of attachment. Hazan 8 Shaver (1987) have demonstrated that individuals' perceptions of their relationship with a romantic partner can be categorized into styles of attachment similar to those of mother-infant attachment conceptualized by Ainsworth at al (1978). One might hypothesize that dating couples who experience violent and other maladaptive conflict resolution behaviors in their relationship may be insecurely attached in their relationship. Also, gathering information about each partners own attachment history by administering an attachment interview (George, Kaplan 8 Main, 1984) may reveal interesting differences between coercive and noncoercive couples. lnforrnation gathered about the current relationship should include some measure of the amount of conflict in the relationship, as well as information concaming what the couple does when they are together on a date (i.e., do they go to movies, stay at home and play board games, how much do they drink alcohol, etc.) and what they do when they are apart (e.g., how often and for how long do they talk on the phone, do they have outside interests, etc.). Further work might include having couples role play and resolve a conflict, real or hypothetical, in a laboratory setting and coding the interaction using a behavioral code such as developed by Gottman (1979), and examining differences In behavior between coercive and noncoercive couples. APPENDICES APPENDIX A Conflict Resolution and Abuse Questionnaire (CRAO) APPENDIX A Conflict Resolution and Abuse Questionnaire (CRAQ) Please read this sheet before completing the questionnaire. As stated In class this questionnaire deals with the topic of behavior between persons who were or are dating, engaged, or married. The questions which follow all address behaviors which have been reported or described by men and women who are involved with each other. While each of the questions refer to activities or actions which have occurred between couples, they may not have occurred between you and your partner. Please answer each question as honestly as possible. Many of the questions concern tepics that are sensitive and personal and several of the questions concern actions that are illegal. Because of the nature of the questions, it is critically important that you make 59 identifying marks anywhere on this questionnaire. Doing this gives you the anonymity which guarantees that you can never be linked to your truthful responses to the items on this questionnaire. If, in the course of completing this questionnaire, you come to a question which you find to be objectionable for some reason. please feel free to skip that question and go on to the next one. If you do not want to complete the questionnaire. regardless of reason. Just return it as described below. If you have questions about the items on this questionnaire. please feel free to contact Dr. Gary Stollak, 353-8877. If, after completing this questionnaire (or at any other time). you believe that you have committed or been the victim of illegal behavior, it is important that you contact one of the following persons or agencies listed below. If. after completing this questionnaire, you wish to talk about your feelings, thoughts or experiences, you may call any of the persons and/or organizations listed below. There are no fees involved in these consultations and. in each case. nobody will ask your name or any other identifying information. Dr. Gary Stollak, Licensed Clinical Psychologist - 353-8877. Council Against Domestic Assault - 372-5572. Please return the questionnaire whether you have completed it or not. according to the instructions on the attached sheet (page 1a). He estimate that it will take about one hour to complete the questionnaire. Reading these statements and completing the following questionnaire shall constitute your informed consent. 59 Pleaee anewer the following queetione about youreelf. ralationehipa. your peat and your current or aoet recent relationehip with your boy/girl friend or. if earried. your huehend/wiie by aarking your reeponeee on theee pagee. Ieportant: So that your privacy can he protected pleeee do not write your neae on thie queetionnaire. 1) 2) 3) 4) 5) h) 7) 8) 9) Uhat ie your age? what ie your een (circle one)? u what ie your ethnic origin (circle one)? white Ilach fliepenic/Latino eeian other How aany yeare of age were you when you had your firet real date? How aany people have you been out with on eora than one date? How aany relationehipe have you ever been involved laeted (indicate nuaher next to each): leee than a aonth to 2 aenthe to 6 aonthe to 12 aonthe to 2 yeare to 3 yeare or eora yeare Oflt‘flfli‘ flow aany relationehipe have you ever been involved in (indicate nueher next to each): dated nonexclueively dated exclueively diecueaed earriage were engaged lived together were aerried in which Check-each of the activitiee that you have ever engaged in: holding hande kieeing on eouth petting ahove waiet petting below waiet eexuel intercouree How aany partnere have you had eexual intercouree with? (Ouaetione 10 to 15 are on the back of the page) 60 10) Are you currently involved in a relationahip (check one)? Yea lo 11) low long have you been in your current relationehip (or how long were you in your eoat recent reletionehip) (check one)? leee than a aonth ----- 1 to 2 aonthe --..- 3 to 6 aonthe __..- 7 to 12 aonthe -..-- 1 to 2 yeare ..--- 3 to 3 yeare ....- 6 or aore yeare ..... 12) which of the following phreeee beet deecribee your current or aoet recent relationehip (check one)? . dating nonexclueively dating exclueively ---.. diecueeing earriege ..--- engage-ant ----- living together ----- aerriage -..-- 13) Check each of the ectivitiae that you have engaged in in your current or aoet recent reletionehip: holding hande ..-.. kieeing on aouth ----- petting above waiet --..- petting below waiet -__-- eeeual interc'ouree ...-.. 14) How often hae your current or aoet recent boy/girl friend or. if harried. huebendlwife been drunk or high on druga in your preaence while you were eober? lever 1 to 4 tieee 5 to 10 tiaee 11 to 20 tieee 21 or aore tiaee 15) How often were you drunk or high on druga in the preaence of your current or eoet recent boy/girl friend or. if aerried. hueband/wifa while helehe wee eober? lever 1 to a tiaee S to 10 tiaee 11 to 20 tiaee 21 or aore tinee (Pleaee go on to next page) a" 61 16) Iow often were you both drunk or high on druga together? Never 1 to e tiaee 5 to 10 tieee 11 to 20 tieea 21 or eore tiaee --- “ Pleaea enawer the following gueetione concerning the parent(e), etep-perentte). or guerdianCe) that you lived with before coaing to college or while you were a teenager. 17) The parent(a). etep-parentte). or guerdian(e) that you lived with before coaing to college or while you were a teenager wea(were) (check one): your biological nether and biological father your biological nether and etep father your biological father and atep aother your anther only your father only your relativee or footer/adoptive parente 18) Education of your nether/feeele parent (check one): gueetion not applicable leee than eigth grade eoee high echool high echool graduate aeae college college graduate graduate or profeeeional 19) Occupation of aother/feaale parent (check one): gueetion not applicable hoaeaeker blue collar white collar profeeeional uneaployed _-----_ 20) If your aother/feaale parent worke outeide the hone. doee ehe work (check one): gueetion not applicable pert tine full tine 21) Age of aotherlfaaale parent (if applicable): (Oueetione 22 to 27 are on the back of eheet) (z ‘ 62 22) Education of your father/hale parent (check one): gueetion not applicable leee than eigth grade acne high achool high achool graduate none college college graduate graduate or profeeeional 23) Occupation of fatherlnale parent (check one): gueation not applicable honenaker blue collar white collar profeeeional unenployed 2e) if your father/nala parent worke outeide the bone, doee he work (check one): gueetion not applicable __..__ part tine ...... full tine _...._ 25) Age of father/nale parent (if applicable): 26) Did you ever nee your parent(e) (Check each itea that appliee). Threaten to hit or throw eonething at each other. Throw. hit. kick. or aneeh eonething. Throw eonething at each other. Pueh. grab. or above each other. Slap each other. hick. bite. or hit the other. heat up the other. . Threaten each other with a gun or knife. Can a gun or knife on each other. 27) Did your parente ever do any of the following thinge to you (check each itea that appliee): Threaten to hit you or throw eonething at you. Throw.aoaething at you. Pueh. grab. or above you. Spank you Slap you. lick. bite. or hit you. beat you up. Threaten you with a gun or knife. “no a gun or knife on you. (Pleaee go on to the next page) 63 MutWadchciflermtitisbmnmplidhym,murmtwmost rant boy/girlfrinid. tr yor current a- mat recent relationship. If it is true as mpliad toyou, mornirmatremtbw/girlfriaud.rmnrrmtrmostrmt relationship circlethe kit: 1’. If it isialaenuipliaa toyw.yororrcitor mat mow/girlfrimdmrm orrmtreutmutnlatWip.circktlwlattr F. liywnnotreportimonanrrent “Wimmtlunltunsasmwmlmthavemplladdrimvnrnmtrantrelatiomhip. oyou migct have felt while that relationship we still wing on. i F l.MutimanwMy/girliriedmnmianMmet-Mpy. ' T F zmreldinmiphmtaprfctnm T F 3.nyboylgirlirmhasalldtlugnlltinl havealweyswmta) lna boy/girlirlem I F 4.linybo/Igrlirieldhuelyfnaltslntuawoedtlun. I F 5. fly boy/girlfriuideidl Wash othw' coupletely. T F QWeandlaqinMnmytwopn-minthiswrldmbe 1’ F 7.Iheve mmmnmwmmwmy alumna T F 8.!va'ymihmglhevewwnybo/lwliriudhnplum T F ulna-acetinnwhuilm miniag-Itmlddictimio-myboy/girliriad T F iOJdlI't ihirtmecouldpuiblybehmpio'timmybw/girlfrinidaulwm werewiihonemothu‘. T F ILHYNHmiDMHNMDWUmitB. T F IZIM'imlembmflhmwimmmMm my/qirlfrwml. I F l3. Whey/girlfriuumpldeiymadmpathiu with myeverymoni T F lilhwemm'ettaimvrelatmeutmia‘amt T F is.limp-minthewrlddtluoppnsitaaMbmavailablamdwllllmtnbe MIWW|DWlmM|WU|u WMOWU'DU. " ‘IIIIllIIIlllllllllllllllllllllll DO 3 is NOT WRITE IN THIS AREA ®O®®®©©O® 9 lo nutter how well a couple gate along. there are tinee OO©®®®OO®® when they diaagree on eager or ninor nattere. get annoyed about eonething the other pereon hae done. or Juet have OO®®®©®O®® epata or fighte becauae they are in a bad need or are tired. or for none other reaeen. Sena couplee have aany OO®®®®®O®® different waye of eettling their differencen. while other couplee have only a few waye of eettling their differencen. ®O®®®©QO®Q we are intereeted in how frequently [9! have uaed each of the following nethede te renolve cenflicta between 199m OOQ®®®®®®® and your 9513.“! or [931 um boy/girl friend or. if canied. your huaband/wife. Pleeee one the following ecale OOQGGQGOGQ to indicate how often you have uaed each of the following netbede of confronting conflicta with hie/her by blackening ®O®®O®®O®® the apppropriate circle after each itea. ©0®®®©®®®O if you find that a guention ie objectionable. leave it blank and go on to the neat gueetien. ®®®©®©©O©® Scale: OQCDGDGOOOCDG o-never 2-dto10tinee 4-21orneretinee 1 I 1 to 3 tinee I I 11 to 20 tinee OOQGGQOOGQ low often have you . . . ®O®®®OOO©© 1) Defanded youreelf verbally. . . . . . . OO©®®OOOOO 2) Ignored the problan. . . . . . . . . 0060900000 2) naked another pereen to help nettle thinge. . . 00609600636) d) land alcohol or druga to nettle conflict. . . ©G®®®O®O®® 5) Gotten and at eonebody elee heceuae of conflict. ®®©®®©®O®® a) Talked to a friend about the problen. . . , . . OO©®®©@O®O 1) Frayed with bin/her or alone. . . . . . . ®O©@®®@O®® 0) bone or aaid eonething to nake bin/her laugh. . . o©©®®®®0®® 9) lode annual advancee toward hie/her. . . . . OQ©®®®®®®® 10) heat for walk. Jagged or worked out. . . . . OO©©®©®O®® 11) Put off talking about the conflict. . . . . ooe®®®©®®® 12> ‘Talked down“ to hie/her. . . . . . . 0093930090 13) lagged at hie/her. . . . . . . . . 0090903000 14) Done eonething to hin/her that left a bruiae. 0900903330 15) Oenied that there wae a problen. . . . . . OOQOGOQ"OC 16) Slapped hinlher on face. . . . . . . . COOL, 130.3300 17) Deetreyed er danaged an object belonging to hie/her. 95:3: : 3 :33;- ‘Q -oa-oc CO .q 0. 9.. Q. 55‘ h-~b .l Oh. oh--.\ I AA a. AAAAAA I (‘in A .45.‘. 'A .. 65 J:- II». .- .--—- - - —A_.--A.. ~— \ 4 Javu vavv ll .) (l i) l ) l l ) ) ‘qA—‘§A ‘ ‘ ‘ g g ‘ ‘ ‘ A A a v .1 v ‘d ‘ 4 cf ‘ J ‘1 in) ‘1’) 20) 21) 33) " 20 as: as) 27) 2e) as) an) an f2) 33 34) 35) 3‘) 37) 3.) 3’) d1) ‘2) O3) 44) Q8) Called hie/her bod nanote). Stoopod foot. lode or given an enouao for what happened. Left the reenlhouae. Infueed to talk about it. Iorgainod or negotiated with bin/her. Changed the topic. lurt bin/her aeverely enough to require attention. Lectured bin/her. Ilaoed hie/her for problen. Apologinod to hie/her. Sowoened hie/bar painfully e.g.. their wriet or are. Slanned the deer. Sulhod or peuted. Shot bin/her with a gun. ) Threotoned to phynically horn bin/her. Argued with hie/her. Agreed that there won a problen. lode fun of or ridiculed hie/her. linicked or nechod bin/her. Spohen or acted eorcaetically to hin/har. Seothad or calned hie/her. Stabbed or cut hie/her with a knife. Igged hie/her on. lecane affectionate towarda hie/her. noted confuood 1.... won wrong. lecone very logical Coiled polioo. ‘91..“ ,0 we ‘0 a. ”on. p-.-) nodica acted like you didn‘t know wt a.-umun-uuwuwuwuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuwun‘uwuu DO j i NOT WRITE IN THIS AREA QGGOGOOOGC 'O” 3 OOGGGGGQGC ea) lade a pernenal verbal attack agninet hie/her. QQQQGOQQCDG d7) Telled or ncrenned at hie/her. . . . . . QQQQQQQQGC 66) Threatened bin/her with a knife. . . . . . @QQQQQQOGC 49) Suggeeted pooeible nolutionn . . . . . QQQQQQOOQC 50) naked bin/h. to help find nolutionn. . . . . QOQG QOOOOC 51) brought up hie/her poet nhortconingn or faulte. QQQGGQOOQC $2) Forgiven hie/her. . . . . . . . . . QQQQQQQQQC 31) Acknowledged hie/her feelinga about the problen. QOQGCCOCOC ed) Ordered hin/hnr around. . . . . . . . QQQQQQFQQC SS) Choked or etraagled bin/her. . . . . . . QQQQQQQ COG; as) licked an object (not hie/her). . . . . . QOQQQQQ QCE S?) licked bin/her. . . . . . . . . . QQQQQQQ:3.:f an) bitten bin/her. . . . . . . . . . 000.33.33.33: SS) Pulled hie/her hair. . . . . . . . . QQQQQQQQQC? 60) Slapped hinlhnr on body. . . . . . . . OOGOQCQCQC‘: 61) lit bin/her with your fiat. . . . . . . QQQQQQQQQC 62) Thrown an object at hie/her. . . . . . . QQQQQQQQQQ 63) Thrown an object but not at hie/her. . . . . QQQQQQQQ QQ I M) Thrown bin/her. . . . . . . . . . QQQQQQQQQQ 6S) lit hie/her with an ebyect. . . . . . . QQQQQQQ QQ : 66) lecane angry at bin/her. . . . . . . . OQQQQQQQQQ 67) Threatened bin/her with a gun. . . . . . OOCJOGCC QQQ The next not of guentionn deal with behaviorn that night OCCCCC : C 3:? occur between two people who are clean to one another. we are intereeted in how frequently you have done each of the following thinge to your snugly: or 5931 flflgfilz boy/girl friend or. if narried. your huebnnd/wifa. Again. uao the following acnla to indicate how often you have done each of the following to bin/her by blackening the apppreprinte circle. Scale: o-never 2-dte10tinee loitoatinee 3-11te20tinee a I 21 or nere tinee (0went1one6lte’5areonthebnckoftheehaot) . — A ‘ .- ' CAAA’ O \Jv‘vawvdv ,a‘,‘-\A«A.\A CCV'V-rvvw-tv AAA" ««««« oL‘p/Kd\;wa\rw/o‘ I O fifl/‘AAAA‘, l V‘v4v\../Vv AA .......... ‘VKVV ........ AAA‘A AAAAAA -- —_..- --- .- 443“). an Aflmudlwd Action (Que! Ummllumly Irunhlmn C) C) (J C) C C) C) () () C) ) C) i) (9 Q) 3 C) (I i) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (D C) ‘ l) C) C) ( C) C) C) C) i) C) C C) () C) ) (D ) () C) k C) ( () (3 () C) C) > ( (, i) () C) () () C) () C) C) C) C) (J C) (D C) i) C) (I) i i) C C) () l) C) ) l ) l t ) l ) v-r Va v v .v v v ‘1 l 3 ) l ) l ) () C) C) (3 C) C) C) C) (D (D C) C) C) C) (D C) C) C) C) C) () C) C) C) () C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (J C) l) C) C) (j l) C) C) C) C) C) ‘ C) C) C) C) () C) (D C) C) C) l) C) l) C) (J C) C) C) l) l) i) l) (3 C) C) C) (J l) C) l ) l) ) l ) l ) ) ') ) AA\\-\- \A 00000 C) C) C) (3 C) (3 C) C) (3 (3 C (J ( t) C l) (, C) C () C) - C) C) (3 C) C) C) ( C) G) (3 () ’1) 67_ lnbnrraeeod hie/her in public. Spoken well of hin/hor in front of othern. Told cruel or neon Jekee about hie/her in their preaence. Gotten upnot when holnhe went out with none eex {time Tried to tell bin/her who hie/her friendn nhould be. Ootten upeet when he/ehe went out with oppoeitn nex friende. benandod that he/ahe account for hie/her eotivitioe anc :tlzely acounod hie/her of having been out with another :::::=; acounod bin/her having one with eenoena elee. Conplained of hie/her flirting with othern. lecueod hie/her of keeping nocrete. Looked through hie/her pernonal belengingn. Spied on bin/her. bellied hinlher. Onnondod hie/her obedience to your whine. been )enleue or eunpicioun of hie/her friende. Told bin/her that he/ehe can‘t get along without you. Oenanded that hnlnhe nerve/wait on you. Told bin/her that helnhe doenn't really underntand what the world in like. Oenanded that helehe not necinlixo with friende. Ordered hie/her around. hie/her Told hie/her that holnhe in attractive. Told bin/her that he/abe in fun to be with. Told bin/her that he/nha in ntupid. Laughed at hie/her feelingn. Told bin/her that you couldn't live without bin/her. Oenplinonted hinlhor on loveneking abilitioo. initiated dincueeien of annual feelinge and/or behavior (Pl-one an on to the port none) V.’ “M-A‘AMUMM“M‘A‘A‘A‘A‘A‘A‘A‘A‘A“‘A‘A“‘A“‘A‘A‘A‘AA“ A“““AA“‘A“A‘A““A‘AA“‘A“““A““‘AA‘AA‘A‘A‘“““A“- -- ‘ 'Il DO i - NOT WRITE 68 IN THIS AREA 101) 102) 103) 104) ion) 106) 107) ion) ioe) iio) 111) 112) 113) 116) 11S) 116) 111) 11a) 11’) 120) 12)) 122) 123) Page S Told hie/her that hie/her lovenaking wen worao than that of othern. Told bin/her that you wanted to have aax with eenoena elee. Called bin/her 'loeao'. 'pronincoue“. or ”aany“. . forced bin/her into annual activity in epite of hie/her pretantn. been unable to tell bin/her that annual activity in undenired. Touched or fondled hie/her genitale in epite of hie/her pretantn. Talked bin/her into annual intarceurao in epite of hie/her pretantn. Phynically forced hie/her to have annual interceurne. conplinnotnd hie/her on appearance. . . . . Told hie/her what in exciting or arouaing about bin/her. Told bin/her that annual feelinge or eotivitioe are 'dirty'. Started a dincueeien about contraception with hie/her. Threatened to leave hie/her if annual activity wan not forthconing. land annual devionn in epite of hie/her pretantn. Told hie/her that halnha won a bad lover. . . Threatened to phyeicnlly horn hie/her if annual activity won not forthconing. forced bin/her to act out behavior portrayed in pornography. Told hie/her that helahe wan ugly. . . . . leld or cuddled bin/her when naked to do no. . . lad intorcourno without contraception in epite of hie/her pretantn. Unfavorably coapnred hie/her to other non/wenon. Told bin/her that holnhe in reepeneible for planning you. Oennnded'aexual activity no part of the relationehip. Tried to aoduco othnrn in front of hie/her. . . lecone eullen or withdrawn if annual activity wan not forthconing. Touched hie/her in public in epite of hie/her pretantn. ' lode hie/her look at pornographic natarialn in epite of hie/her pretantn. Kieead hie/her in epite of hie/her pretantn. . . (Plneaa go to the back of the page for the next part) OOOOGOGOGC OOQGGQGOGC OO©®®OOO©C OOQGGOCOOC QOQQGGOOOC OOQQGOOOOC OOQGQOQOGC GOGGQOOOOC OOQGQCGOGC OOGGGOQOQC 00096“ OCOOC OOQOQOOOJC OOOGGOOOCC OOOGQOOOO: OOQCGQGOOC AAAAAA ...... v d'v" to v ‘75 U I; an Aflumdlwr Achon (Qua! Opportunity Inummun .-I. Pill-m), .5- '-'.I'r' Ull'i I" A, "I ll \ C) () C) C) C) C) C) () () a- .- an--- ') e- —-a C) 08 C) C) C) C) C) C) ) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) I) O C) C) C) C) C) C) (D C) () C) (3 U. (DC 0000( C) C) C) C) . (J ” () l C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) () C) - C) C) C) C) C) () C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) - C) C) C) (D C) C) (D C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (J C) C) C) (D C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) - ) C) C) C) Q C) C) C) (D C) C) C) C) C) C) \ ( ) I C) C) C) C) I) I C) C CI C (D C) C l) C h N C) I C) C C) C C) (7 C) C) C) C) ‘ C) C) C) C) C) C) C) () C) C) C) ' C) ‘ C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) i) C) C) C) C) K I i I" I l ( ‘a-‘a‘ \ C C C ( C C C C) C) C) ) ) l I I l ) J l l I I I ‘ - .1 - a. s IDS! huaband/wife haa renolve conflictn between the each of the following nethede of confronting conflictn wit) you by blackening the apppreprinte circle after each itea. If you are not aura of hie/her behavior. beet entinnte. Scale: O I never 2 I d to 10 tinee d I 21 or none tine. 1 I 1 to S tinee S I 11 to 20 tinee I00 1) 2) I) 6) 5) 6) 7) I) S) 10) 11) 12) 13) 1C) 15) 16) 17) 1.) ‘19) 21) 69 IN THIS - - 3""53 .- ken“ . \ .53 J- b h brain"; Page 6 we are intereeted in how frequently your QQBBIII 03 [[93]: boy/girl friend or. if narried. you: uaed each of the following nethede tI llltfllBSILE Ind 199- PIIIII “I following ncnle to indicate how often he/nhe hen unec pleaae give you) often hen he/ahe . . . Oefended hie/her aelf verbally. Ignored the problen. baked another perncn to help nettle thinge. land alcohol or druga to nettle conflict. Gotten and at aonebody elne becauae of conflict. Talked to a friend about the problen. Frayed with you or alone. been or aeid eonething to nake you laugh. lode annual advancea toward you. lent for wolk. Jogged or worked out. Put of! talking about the conflict. “Talked down” to you. lagged at you. been eonething to you that left a bruiao. Denied that there won a problen. Slapped you on face. Ooetreyed or danaged an object belonging to you. Called you had naneta). Stopped feet. lode or given an encuan for what happened. Left the roen/houae. (Pleaae go on to the next page) DO i 6 NOT WRITE 70 IN THIS AREA 0000000366 "II 7 OGGOGGOOCDC 22) lafuaed to talk about it. . .. . . . . . OOCDOGOCDOGC 23) lergained or negotiated with you. . . . . . OOQOQQA‘COC 20 one.“ the topic. . . . . . . . . CDCDCDCDCDOCCOC 23) sort. you aeverely enough to require aedical attention. OOQOQCOOOC 2e) Lectured you. . . . . . . . . . . OOGOGGOOOC 27) Ileaed you for the problen. . . . . . . OOCDOGOCCOC 2n Apologired u. you. . . . . . . . . ©0®®©coooc§ 2a) Squeeaed m painfully ..... your mu or are. ooooeocooc; so) 91...... a. door. . . . . . . . . ooooocococg 31) Sulhed or pouted. . . . . . . . . OCCCC’CCC-CC; 32) Shot you with a gun. . . . . . . . . OOCOGCCCCCE a) Threatened to phyeicnlly here you. . . . . OO©C=CCQ":C' 34) Argued with you. . . . . . . . . . COOS); ‘ :C 3: as) Acuocleceee that there wee a problen. . . . OOGOCCSCQC as) Iade fun of or ridiculed you. . . . . . 006000333: 37) Iiaiched or nocked you. . . . . . . . OOOOOCCCCC 3a) Spoken or acted earcaatically cocci-cc you. . . OOQOCCCCCC 39) soothed or calaed you. . . . . . . . 0060006300 40) Stabbed er out you with a knife. . . . . . OOOOGOOOOC d1) lgged you on. . . . . . . . . . . OOQOCOOOGC e2) Iecaae affectionate with you. . . . . . . OOOQCCC SOC a) noted confuaed i.e.. acted like lie/aha didn‘t know what occeccccoc waa 02”,: M) Iecoae very logical. . . . . . . . . OVCQSCCCCC es) Called police. . . . . . . . . . owe-3;: : : 3: ea) Iadeperaonal verbal attachoayou. . . . . QQCC::C:C: intellecci-ecreececezyou. . . . . . . bbc:‘;;::;i ea) Threatened you with a knife. . . . . . . QCQC333 : j :; 49) Suggeated poaaible eolutiona . . . . . . “““““““ : (OuaationaSOto'IOarethebackoftheaheet) DO NOT am 319......“ .. m. . .5. mxxmvw his) .. .f.. C. .-, C r: C J C) "\ Id C j) C) C) C-) C) CC) 1) C) C) C) C) C) C) C" C) C) C)’() ) C) C C) C) C) C) ) C) C) C) C) C) C) "IJJ‘.’ ~A~AA A a -- .- A -\ a An. A q C) C) C C) C) C n "(u/M," .')'I)’l’[l.lv ~I (._) C) C) C) mu.- 4: l) u. (44,-”Hip, n .- ‘4 v a' -A‘.‘A§ ‘ - \ a s . -‘an-s . A s ‘ a C) C) C) C;) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) U C) C) C) C) Q) ‘00 C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) J C) A C) 4““ C) C) \_I C) C) C) C) Q C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) ' C) C) C) C) ) C) C) 4“ C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) «R ‘ ‘ s s A a \.« e.‘q C) C) 71 WHITE IN 8 “I THIS - - X; Page 0 SO) aaknd you to help find eolutiona. 51) brought up your pant ahortconingn or faulta. S2) forgiven you. 53) acknowledged your fenlinga about the problen. so) Ordered you around. SS) Choked or atrangled you Sh) licked an object (not you). $1) licked you. on) bitten you. 3’) 'ulled your hair. 00) Slapped on your body. 61) lit you with hie/her flat. ‘2) Thrown an object at you. CS) Thrown an ob3ect but not at you. 04) Thrown you. 63) hit you with an object. ‘6) banana angry at you. 67) Threatened you with a gun. The newt net of gunntione deal with behaviorn that nigh occur between two people who are clone to one another. 0» are intereeted in how frequently your GIBBIII or In! [19:11 boy/girl friend or. if harried. your huaband/wif- haa done each of the following thinge to 199. Again. ua- thn following ncnle to indicate how often halnhe hen don. each of the following to you by blackening the apppropriat circle. Scale: 0 I never 2 I d to 10 tinee e I 21 or nore tinee 1 I l to S tinee 3 I 11 to 20 tinee low often han helahe . . . ea) lnbarrannnd you in public. 69) Spoken well of you in front of othern. 70) Told cruel or nean Joken about you in your preaence. l J ‘72 D0 — — NOT WRITE IN THIS AREA ’1) ’2) 3) 4) 5) '6) ‘7) b) '9) .0) Cl) )2) 3) 0) -3) «SD .7) A» S) ‘0) Cl) ‘2) '3) 4) S) to) 17) 8) 'ege S Gotten upaot when you went out with none new frienda. Tried to tell you who your frieodn nhould be. . Gotten upoet when you went out with. oppooite non frienda. Denanded that you account for your activitieo and tine. Talaely acounod you of having been out with another pornon. Tolnely acounod you having non with aoneone elae. Coaplained that you were flirting with othern. . hocuned you of keeping eocrotn. . . . . . Looked through your parnonal belonginga. . . . Spied on you. . . . .' . . . . . bullied you. . . . . . . . . . . Doaandod your ......... to hie/her whine. . . bean aeoleua or auapicioua of your frienda. . . Told you that helabe can’t get along without you. Dooebdod that you borvoiwoit on bin/bur. . . . Told you that you don't roally underntand what the world in llko. Donanded that you not aocialiae with your frienda. Ordarad you around. . . . . . . . . Told you that you are attractive. . . . . . Told you that you are fun to be with. . . . . Told you that you are ntupid. . . . . . . Laughed at your feelingn. . . . . . . . Told you that you couldn't live without bin/her.- coaplinented you on your lovonaking abilitiee. lnitiatad diaouaaion of annual feelinga and/or behavior with you. Told you that'your lovenaking woe worne than that of othern. Told you that he/nhe wanted to have non with ncaoone elae. Called you ”looee'. 'proaiecoun'. or ”aany“. . . (Mention. Q. fan 1” .9. III- ft- he-h'nr ov- .. -. 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 00000 000{ 0000000 ' 0000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 0000000000 I f" \UC 00 000 MSU): on Affamuhve Aaron/(qua! 0000! “may Inuvlulmn ~ ‘I l) (D C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (.1) (J C_) C) . C) C) C) C.) C) C) C) () C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (D C) - C) ? C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (D C) C) C) C) _) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (D C) C) C) C) C) C.) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) () C) C) Z C) “ C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (D (J C) C C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) '“ C) C) C) (D C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) -‘RQAAAA C) C C) C) C) C) C) ) ) I I ) ) C J (J C) C) C) C) C) C) (D C) C) (3 C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) Q C) C) C) (3 C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) C) (p) C) C) u() .wDCD C) C) I ‘) C) P C) C) C) C) ” 100) 101) 10B) 100) 100) 100) 10‘) 107) 100) 100) 110) 111) 112) 113) 110) 115) 110) 117) 110) 11’) 120) 121) 122) ‘12,) 73 \\\\\\\V\\\\\ “ “AA““AA “M “ feroea you late aeuual aotlvlty la aplte or you preteeta. beau unable to tell you tbat aeuual aotlvlty la uodealree. Touobeelteeelee your gealtala lo aplte or your erotaata. Talked you late aeuual lataroouraa la aolte or your mua ’byaleally loroee you to baye aeuual loteroourae. Coeellaeateo you on your aeoeeraaoe. Told you ubat la euoltlo. or aroualo. about you. Told you tbat aeuual teella'a or aotlvltlee are .M'- e Itartei a dlaouaalea about eoatreoeotloo ultb you. fbreeteoeo to leave you 1! aeuual aotlvlty uaa not lertbooelag. Deed aeuual earloea lo aplte or your proteeta. Told you tbet you uere a bee lever. Tbreateoea ta abyaloally bare you ll aerual aetlvlty uea not tortboealag. 'leroee you to act out bebavler portrayed ln DO'IOOrODhY- Told you tbat you uere ugly. Ield or euodlea you ubeo aabea to do ea. lad lateroouree ultbeut oeotraoeptleo lo aplta or your ta. lulayerably eeeperea you to otber uoaeo/aeo. Told you tbet you are reeeeoalbla (or pleealoo blelber. Deeaaoea aerual aotlylty aa part or tba relatlooablp. Trlee to eeouoe otbera lo {rout oi you. leee-a aullea er ultberaue l: aeuual aotlvlty uaa not fortbeoela'. ?ouebee you lo yubllo la aolta or your protaeta. leee you look et poroegraeblo aaterlala lo epite or your proteata. ‘ llaaeo you lo aplta oi your protaata. Tbaak you for eoeoletlo. tbla aurvey ”WM-“Muvuuuuuu“uuuusouuuuuu‘cuuuuwu“uwuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuu APPENDIX 8 Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) (Form A) APPENDIX B Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) (Form A) Hutch-Mo!!hIapyeu“IhavadeaaotaayaubaiaooaflIflet~aamatwIIhyouthI‘u.\Vc mum. ymtowmmmtmumwmm mound-ounctmihhiuhufiwd. Housman-humour“ onudaflhutblaplluu‘bdevlanhovhouofloa bazyouddluhuyoot 0' New: I - Once that you: 2 ' “Duo 0: than timcs J ' Often. but Ia: than one: a month 4 - About one: a mouth 5 ' Men than once a month " I a. IMIoMIhhuunhuvdyaI-Iyn ........................ 0 I 2 J 4 S uwmmmuuuvayduy............. ................. o I 2,343 c.Gotinfotaudoatobuokup-yuduonhlap........ ................ 0 I 2 3 4 S 4. amount: nmuudutohdpmdulbluphtuuto)” ............... .. o I 2 3 4 S o. Argued homily butMtofyuIIIa. ..... . ................... 0 I 2 3 4 S I. YcIchu ocimuIIod .......... . ......................... 0 I 2 3 4 S a. Sultcda ornfluudtoldkabouul. ................. 0 I 2 3 4 S h. Stamped ouloflha mom . . ................................ 0 I 2 3 4 S I. mewmmmwutnotutuyvifa)otuuuhodsoaown¢ ............. ,.. 0 I 2 3 4 S tThruuoodtohItatbrwmatha... .................. 0 I 2 3 4 S .TIIMWIIIIIIIO! my“: ....... ...................... 0 I 214 5 I Pushednabbcdot Ilet .......................... ....... 0 I 2 3 4 S cu.Iliumuicdwhiflhuhutaotwflhuayomo; ...... ........... .. 0 I 2 J 4 S a. III:(o:IrI¢dIohII)haruvIIhlo.-IhI-;hud...'. ..................... 0 I 2 J 4 S 74 APPENDIX C Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) (Form N) APPENDIX C Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) (Form N) No tuuttut boo vol a couple pt- quuo. than on than: when they elae.-c on Not decisions. pt annoyed about touted“; hmeuMb-nmuotfldmmthoy‘nianbudmmulottimd otfo: «out way: of trying to settle that Mm I'nt going to «and a list of to. thing that you and you: (husbandlpattnor) might have done when you Ind a dispute. and wound first IIbeyou to tell nefotoaeboaobovot’touyou ddttin the putyw. I 2 I 3' I I I MWICIMA 0.18 0.79 0.8" II. dent- Husbandll‘artnet- live: In at Yen In I’utYeat Happened i- I- “ 3 3 2 ' a a 23 s 2 - 2%. s .3: 8 §:;5* 5%:252 f 8 “avg:- .25 ”gt-32'3- .. 'c gag-3.233 §5¢3¢:§o $353 token-“WWW '0I234S6X 0I234$6X 12x b. Gottnromttoutobuekup , (rout/hisItidooIthiw 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X I 2 X c. Itoudttiaotttiodtobn'ngh mtohflputththtn’ 0I234S6X onzztsox I2X I. Insultodonmatthoothaoot 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X I 2 X e. Sulkod uao/o: Infused to talk about“ 0I234S6X 0I23456X 12x r. Stompdoutotthutoo-ot boo-Howard) 0I234$6X 0I23456X sz {Cried 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X 0 I '2 3 4 S 6 X I 2 X DIdouuidmtoqIII Metham- ~0I234S6X 01234sox sz I. Threatemdtohttetthluveooa- thingnttheotbuouo o I 2 3 4 3 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 X I2 X . Th anuhodothtteruekd . Juana; 0I234S6X 0I234$6X I2X bfhmsotnethlnuttboothuono 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X I 2 X I. Footed. Mamas. cannon:n ‘ O I 2 3 4 3 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 X I 2 X nt.SIupnedtheotherona 0 I 2 3 4 3 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 X I2 X n. Kicked.btt.othttvlthuflu 0 I 2 3 4 3 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 X I 2 X o. Hitottnedtohttwlthtontethln; 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X I 2 X p. IIeatuptheothatono 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 X I 2 X o.1‘hmtenodvtthahnlfeotgun 0 I 2 3 4 3 6 X 0 I 2 3 4 5'6 X I 2 X t. Usedaknifeotgun 0 I 2 3 4 5 6 X t 0 I 2 3 4 S 6 X I 2 X tOtheHPROBE): 0I234S6X 0I234S6X I2X 79. And what about you! (hub-adiputtnet)? Tell me how often he (ITEM) In the past yen. -' For each Itetn dtelod etthot “Never” or “Don't Known" for BOTH respondent and partnet, ash; 80. Old you or you: (husband/partner) ever (ITI-ZM)? 75 APPENDIX D Index of Spouse Abuse (ISA) APPENDIX D Index of Spouse Abuse (ISA) INDEX OF SI‘OUSE ABUSL (ISA) This questionnaire is designed to measure the degree of abuse you has e esperienced in your relationship with your partner. It is not a tee. so there are no right or wruny answers. Answer each item as carefully and accurately as you can by placing a number beside each one as follows: I Never 2 Rarely J Occasionally . 4 Frequently 3 Very Frequently Please begin. . My partner belittles me. (I) . My partner demands obedience to his whims. (I1) My partner becomes surly and angry if I tell him he is drinking too much. (I5) My partner makes me perform sea acts that I do not enjoy or like. (50) My partner becomes very upset if dinner. housework or laundry is not done when he thinks it should be. (4) My partner is yeast and suspicious of my friends. (1) My partner punches me with his fists. (75) My partner tells me I am ugly and unattractive. (26) My partner tells me I really couldn‘t manage or take care of myself without him. (I) I0. My partner acts like I am his personal servant. (20) II. My partner insults or shames me in front of Others. (4|) l2. My partner becomes very angry if I disagree with his point of sicw. (IS) I3. My partner threatens me with a weapon. (82) I4. My partner is stingy in giving me enough money to run our home. (I2) IS. My partner belittlcs ntc intellectually. (20) I6. My partner demands that I stay home to take care of the children. (I4) I7. My partner beats me so badly that I must seek medical help. (91) II. My partner feels that I should not work or go to school. (2|) I9. My partner is not a kind person. (I3) 20. My partner does not want me to socialize with my female friends. (I!) 2|. My partner demands sea whether I want it or not. (32) 22. My partner screams and yells at me. (31) 3. My partner slaps me around my face and head. (80) 23 26 9999 999- . My partner becomes abusive when he drinks. (6S) . My partner orders the around. (29) . My partner has notr'espeet for my feelings. (39) ° 27. My partner acts like a bully towards me. (44) 21. My partner frightens me. (33) 29. My partner treats me like a dunee. (29) 30. My partner acts like he would like to kill me. (80) Copyright 0 Sally R. McIntosh and Walter W. Hudson. I978. P: 3. 4. 1. I3. l7. 22-24. 27. 21. 30. NP: I. 2. S. 6. 1-I2. I4-l6. I8-2I. 25. 26. 29. 76 li||||||||lllllllllilllllli llll ‘Al'-'. “-2 APPENDIX E Table 17: Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 APPENDIX E Frequency of Responses to CRAQ Items 1-67 Table 17 Frequency of Responses to CRAQ Items 1-67 Frecpency Iem Never 1t03Ttmes 4to10Ttmes 11t020Ttmes 21orMore AtLeastOnce #1 4.2 18.2 25.3 20.3 31.9 95.7 Detended yourself verbally #2 20.5 28.6 27.4 15.3 8.3 79.5 Ignored the problem 43 45.3 32.0 15.2 4.7 2.9 54.8 Asked another to help #4 79.6 12.6 4.1 2.1 1.7 20.5 Used alcohol or drugs #5 28.3 38.7 19.9 7.8 5.3 71.7 Gotten mad at somebody else #8 5.7 21.7 28.7 20.8 23.2 94.4 Talked to a friend #7 62.6 14.2 8.8 5.3 9.1 37.4 Frayed #8 3.3 10.4 13.2 19.4 53.7 96.7 Done or said some- thing to make the other laugh 77 78 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAQ Items 1-67 Frquency Item Never 1t03firnes 4to10Ttmes 11t020Ttmes 21orMore AtLeastOnce #9 19.8 19.6 16.7 11.8 32.1 80.2 sexual advances #10 10.7 24.7 23.6 18.1 22.9 89.3 Well tor wak' pggedi OfWOl‘kOd out #11 19.3 35.1 25.6 13.1 6.9 60.7 taldngabout tlteoomliot #12 43.9 28.3 16.1 8.0 3.8 56.2 Talked down to him/her #13 30.9 31.4 19.2 11.7 6.6 69.1 Nagged athim/her #14 83.2 13.1 2.7 0.8 0.3 16.9 something that lettabrulse #15 45.7 37.4 9.9 4.4 2.6 54.3 Denied that there was a 79 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 Frecpency Item Never 11031‘tmes 4t010Ttmes 11to201'tmes 21 orMore AtLeastOnce #18 88.1 9.7 1.2 0.9 0.2 12.0 Slapped him/her on lace #17 87.5 11.0 1.2 0.2 0.2 12.6 Destroyed or damaged an object belonging to hIther #18 42.0 29.3 15.0 7.6 6.2 58.1 Called htmlher bad names #19 62.5 22.4 10.0 3.0 2.1 37.5 Stamped feet #20 11.2 38.1 29.5 13.5 7.7 88.8 Made or given an excuse #21 29.2 39.0 16.8 10.6 4.5 70.9 Left the IOOfl'l/I'DUSO #22 43.1 32.5 13.7 6.9 3.8 56.9 Retused to 13k #23 23.0 33.2 25.8 10.7 7.3 77.0 Bargained or negotiated Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 80 Mm Never 1t03Ttmes 4to10Ttmes 1110201'tmes 21 orMore AtLeastOnce Fremency #24 the topic Hurt severely enough to require thedoor #31 Sulsed orpouted Shot them 23.3 98.9 26.4 26.3 3.8 79.8 46.2 21.6 37.5 0.5 36.0 14.9 30.3 0.2 0.3 20.5 21.0 25.2 3.3 14.5 20.0 0.0 10.4 0.2 10.9 10.6 24.5 1.5 5.8 12.6 0.2 5.4 0.2 8.7 6.2 23.0 0.5 3.2 12.1 0.0 76.7 1.2 73.6 73.6 96.3 20.2 78.4 0.4 re“- “ 81 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 quiency lam Never 1t03Times 4t0101'tmes 1110201'tmes 21orMore AtLeastOnce #33 90.9 6.5 1.8 0.3 0.5 9.1 Threatened to Wisely hmn #84 10.9 23.9 20.0 16.6 28.6 89.1 Argued #35 7.3 32.2 25.6 19.1 15.9 92.8 Agreed there was a problem #38 42.7 31 .3 15.4 6.3 4.2 57.2 Made fun of them #37 39.2 31.1 18.1 6.0 5.6 60.8 Mimicked or mocked #38 17.7 32.4 22.5 14.4 13.0 82.3 Spoken F‘- #39 6.5 19.6 23.6 23.1 27.2 93.5 orcaltrted them #40 98.9 0.5 0.5 0.2 0.0 1.2 (PVT-’4 .._-. - 1” Of cut #41 48.6 25.3 14.1 7.9 4.1 51.4 himlher on 82 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 Frecpertcy lam Never 1to3Ttmes 4to10T1mes 11t020'l'tmes 21 orMore AtLeastOnce #42 5.7 17.1 14.7 18.8 43.7 94.3 Became wectbnate #43 27.6 33.8 20.7 13.3 4.5 72.3 Acted confused #44 8.6 24.5 26.6 21.8 18.5 91.4 Became VOW beleal #45 98.9 0.6 0.0 0.3 0.2 1.1 Caled police #46 46.1 28.5 14.2 6.2 5.1 54.0 Made a personal vetbal attack #47 35.8 30.1 14.7 9.3 10.1 64.2 Yelled or screamed #48 98.6 0.8 0.2 0.5 0.0 1.5 Threatened wih a knife #49 4.5 23.8 27.7 23.9 20.2 95.6 Suggested possible 83 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAQ Items 1-67 Fremertcy Item Never 1t03T1mes 4t010Ttmes 11t020Times 21orMore AtLeastOnce #50 7.7 26.8 26.8 20.2 16.6 92.4 sokttiens #51 31.6 31.5 18.2 11.4 7.2 68.3 Brought UP 938! laults #52 3.6 20.2 23.7 22.9 29.6 96.4 Forgiven #53 2.6 17.5 24.1 28.0 27.8 97.4 Acknowledged their teellnos #54 50.7 27.4 12.0 6.3 3.6 49.3 them around #55 98.0 1.7 0.2 0.2 0.0 2.1 Choked orstrangled #56 72.9 16.4 6.8 3.0 0.9 27.1 Kicked an object #57 95.5 3.2 0.9 0.5 0.0 4.6 them 84 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency 01 Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 Fremency Item Never 1t03'l'tmes 4to10Ttmes 11t020'l'1mes 21orMore AtLeastOnce #58 94.3 3.8 0.9 0.5 0.6 5.8 Bitten them #59 91.9 5.4 1.2 1.4 0.2 8.2 PUIled their hair #60 79.0 13.4 4.8 1.4 1.4 21.0 Slapped them onbcdy #61 89.0 7.2 2.1 1.5 0.2 11.0 Hi them with yourtlst #62 91.0 5.1 3.2 0.6 0.2 9.1 anobiect atthem #63 74.5 17.6 5.7 1.4 0.8 25.5 anobiect (notatthem) #64 97.4 2.4 0.2 0.0 0.0 2.6 them #65 92.0 4.5 2.6 0.3 0.6 8.0 Hit them withan 9"”- 85 Table 17 (Continued) Frequency of Responses to CRAO Items 1-67 Frecpency Rem Never 1t031'1mes 4to10Ttmes 11to20‘I'tmes 21orMore AtLeastOnce #66 16.6 24.1 17.9 20.0 21.4 83.4 Became am am #67 99.4 0.5 0.0 0.2 0.0 0.7 Threatened them with agun Note. N-671 fl APPENDIX F Figure 1: Some Plot of Eigenvalues the nvel ues APPENDIX F Scree Plot of Eigenvalues N O 040)“) 35040-0) ‘00 «wmouodm .» .1. .. i Number of factors Figure I. Scree plot of eigenvalues. 4r APPENDIX G Tables 18—24: Items and Item Loadings of CRAQ Factors APPENDIX G Items and Item Loadings of CRAQ Factors Table 18 Appropriate/Generic Factor of the CRAQ Item number Item Factor loading 53 Acknowledged his/her feelings about the problem .815 49 Suggested possible solutions .808 50 Asked him/her to help find solutions .784 52 Forgiven him/her .756 35 Agreed that there was a problem .755 28 Apologized to him/her .645 34 Argued with him/her .592 39 Soothed or calmed him/her .516 1 Detended yourself verbally .506 66 Became angry at him/her .453 47 Yelled or screamed at him/her .426 44 Became very logical .423 31 Sulked or pouted .399 23 Bargained or negotiated with him/her .384 87 88 Table 19 Moderate Violence Factor of the CRAQ Item number Item Factor loading 59 Pulled his/her hair .834 57 Kicked him/her .801 61 Hit him/her with your fist .788 60 Slapped him/her on body .752 58 Bitten him/her .673 62 Thrown an object at him/her .669 16 Slapped him/her on face .634 65 Hit him/her with an object .617 33 Threatened to physically harm him/her .606 29 Squeezed him/her painfully e.g.. their wrist or arm .399 55 Choked or strangled him/her .353 14 Done something to him/her that left a bruise .312 89 Table 20 Avoid Factor of the CRAQ Item number Item Factor loading 2 Ignored the problem .775 11 Put off talking about the conflict .767 22 Refused to talk about it .694 15 Denied that there was a problem .684 24 Changed the topic . .659 43 Acted confused i.e., acted like you didn't know what was going on .476 5 Gotten mad at somebody else because of conflict .422 3 Asked another person to help settle things .392 21 Left the room/house .360 Table 21 Severe Violence Factor of the CRAO Item number Item Factor loading 48 Threatened him/her with a knife .809 25 Hurt him/her severely enough to require medical attention .720 40 Stabbed or cut him/her with a knife .642 32 Shot him/her with a gun .619 67 Threatened him/her with a gun .351 9 0 Table 22 Distraction Factor of the CRAQ Item number Item Factor loading 8 Done or said something to make him/her laugh .815 9 Made sexual advances toward him/her .780 42 Became affectionate towards him/her .777 10 Went for walk, jogged or worked out .577 39 Soothed or calmed him/her .475 Table 23 Verbal Attack/Coercion Factor of the CRAO Item number Item Factor loading 36 Made fun of or ridiculed him/her -.74e ' 54 Ordered him/her around -.665 37 Mimicked or mocked him/her -.648 12 Talked down to him/her -.643 46 Made a personal verbal attack against him/her -.613 38 Spoken or acted sarcastically to him/her -.595 In: 51 Brought up his/her past shortcomings or faults -.577 41 Egged him/her on -.553 27 Blamed him/her for the problem -.551 13 Nagged at him/her -.520 26 Lectured him/her -.495 47 Yelled or screamed at him/her -.424 18 Called him/her bad name(s) -.412 91 Table 24 Symbolic/Indirect Violence Factor of the CRAQ Item number Item Factor loading 56 Kicked an object (not him/her) 63 Thrown an object but not at him/her 30 Slammed the door 17 - Destroyed or damaged an object belonging to him/her -.671 -.645 -.489 -.344 # IQ." APPENDIX H Figure 2: Plot of Group Centroids on Two Discriminant Functions 1;“ Second discriminant function APPENDIX H Plot of Group Centroids on Two Discriminant Functions m N d . . Belligerent / Active Poettive/Average a o e lnexperienced ° '1' Coercive/ Active (\l I m I -3 -2 ~1 0 1 2 3 First discriminant function Figure 2. Plot of four group centroids on two discriminant functions derived from hupothesized predictors of dating coercion. 92 LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Ainsworth, M. D. S. Blehar M. C. Waters, E. &Wall, S. (1978). 33mm WWW Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum. Arias. l- 8- O'Leary. K- D- (1984). WWW ' ’ ' r ' . Paper presented at the 18th annual convention of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Philadelphia. Bandura. A. (1973). Aggression; AsssisIJsaminnsnaLxsis Englewood Cliffs. N. J.: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. Ross, D. 81 Ross. S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. WW3) Esxsanscx. 63. 575-582 Bandura. A.. & Walters. Ft. (1963). SecaUsaminnandnstssnalibt W. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Bernard, M. L, & Bernard. J. L (1983). Violent intimacy: The family as a model for love relationships. W, 32, 283-286. Carroll, J. C. (1977). The intergenerational transmission of family violence: The long-term effects of aggressive behavior. W,1 289—299. Cane" R (1978) IbsssisnflflsusssttastoLanaIstanbsnaxicraLandms W. New York. Plenum Press. Curtis. W. J. Maguin, E. &Stollak, G. (1987). W .Paper presented at the annual convention of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago. Dixon. W. J. (1981). W. Bekeley, CA.: University of Califomia Press. Dobash. R. E. 8: Dobash. R. P. (1979). MislsnssasainsmixsssAsasescains: W. New York: Free Press. 93 ll." 94 Edmonds, V. H. (1967). Maritalconventionalization: Definition and measurament MW 23 531533 Edmonds, V. H. W1thers, G., 8. Dibatista, B. (1972). Adjustment, conservatism, and marital conventionalization. WM 34,, 96-1 03. Gelles, R. J. (1974). W. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage Publications. George. 0.. Kaplan. M. 8. Main, M. (1984)- AnacnmsnLintsniisinLtnLadults Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley. Gottman. J. M. (1979). Mantaumsractinmfixnsnmsntaljmstinatinns New York: Academic Press. Hartup, W. W. (1977). Peerinteraction and the behavioral development of the individual child. In E. Mavis Hetherington and Ross D. Parke (Eds) Qnmsmnnranusadinssinsnildssxsbslssx (pp 345- -354) New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Hazan, C. 81 Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process W 62. 511-524 . Hudson, W. W. 6Mclntosh, S. R. (1981). The assessment of spouse abuse: Two quantifiable dimensions JnumsLsLMacnanaandJnsEamilx. .43. 873-885. Jorgensen, S. R. (1977). Social class heterogamy, status striving, and perceptions of marital conflict: A partial replication and revision of Pearlin' 5 contingency hypothesis JournaLsLMsmsnsandeLEamih: 69. 653-661 Klecka, W. R. (1980).)2isgfim1n30Lanalysjs. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. Lane, K. E., 6Gwartney-Gibbs, P. A. (1985). Violence in the context of dating and sex JsumalsLEamilLlssuss. 6. 45-59 Laner, M. R. (1983). Courtship abuse and aggression: Contextual aspects. SosisbcisaLSnsstnim 3. 69-83 Laner, R. L., &Thompson, J. (1982). Abuse and aggression in courting couples. DsitianLBsnaxiscAnJmsLdissinlinanulsumaI 3. 229-244 Maccoby. E., & Jacklin, C. (1974). W333. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press. 95 Maccoby, E., 8 Jacklin, C. (1980). Sex differences in aggression: A rejoinder and reprise. Qnildnsxsbnmsm. 51. 964-980. Maguin, E., Curtis, W. J. 8 Stollak, G. (1987). WWW goggles. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association, New York Makepeace, J. M. (1983). Life events stress and courtship violence. Eamily leatisns. 62. 101 -109. Matthews, W. (1984). Violence in college couples. WW3), 1.8. 150-158. McCord, W., McCord, J. 8 Howard, A. (1961). Familial correlates of aggression In nondslinqusnt male children JnumsLnLAbnnnnaLandfissiaLEsysnnlscx 32, 79-93. McCord, J. McCord, W. 8 Howard, A. (1963). Family interaction as antecedent to the direction of male aggressiveness. WWW Esxsbclncx. 66. 239-242 Meichenbaum. D. (1977). WWW 3123:3333. New York: Plenum Press. Nie, N. Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. Steinbrenner, K. 8Bent, D. (1975). 5131(3))33) nachacslcunasnsiaLssisnssslrsv sd--) New York: McGraw-Hill NunnaIIy, J. C. (1978). W (rev. ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Owens, D. M., 8 Straus, M. A. (1975). The social structure of violence in PE childhood and approval of violence as an adult. W. 1. 193-211. Pagelow, M. D. (1981) WWW Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. W Patterson, G. R. (1976). The aggressive child: Victim and architect ofa coercive system. In E. J. Mash, LA. Hamerlynck, 8L. C. Handy (Eds.,) M31191 W New York: Brunner/Mazel. Peterson, D. (1983). Conflict. In H. H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christenesen, J. Harvey, T. Huston, G. Levinger, E. McCIintock, L. A. Peplau, 8 D. Peterson (Eds). Insnsxstinlsoxsisbsuslstisnsnips (pp 360-396). San Francisco: Freeman. 95 Maccoby, E., 8 Jacklin, C. (1980). Sex differences in aggression: A rejoinder and reprise. W, 51, 964-980. M39Uin. E., Curtis, W. J. 8 Stollak, G. (1987). WWW muglgs. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Psychological Association, New York. Makepeace, J. M. (1983). Life events stress and courtship violence. Eamily 56161193232. 101- 109 Matthews, W. (1984). Violence in college couples. W, 18, 150-158. McCord, W. McCord, J, 8 Howard, A. (1961). Famtltal correlates of aggressmn in nondelinquent male children. Wm, 52, 79- 93. McCord, J, McCord, W., 8 Howard, A. (1963). Family interaction as antecedent to the direction of male aggressiveness. WW Balancing! 6.6. 239 242 Meichenbaum. D. (1977)- WWW 333323311. New York: Plenum Press. Nie, N. Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J., Steinbrenner, K. 8Bent, D. (1975). 313151133) naskansinntnssoctaLssisnssslrsv 9d--) New York McGraw-Hill Nunnally, J. C. (1978). W (rev. ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Owens, D. M., 8 Straus, M. A. (1975). The social structure of violence in childhood and approval of violence as an adult. WW, 1, 193-211. PageIOW. M. D. (1981) WWW Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. Patterson, G. R. (1976). The aggressive child. Victim and architect ofaooercive system. In E. J. Mash, L. A. Hamerlynck, 8 L. C. Handy (Eds.,) 53113119) W. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Peterson, D. (1983). Conflict. In H. H. Kelley, E. Berscheid, A. Christenesen, J. Harvey, T. Huston, G. Levinger, E. McClintock, L. A. Peplau, 8 D. Peterson (Eds). Insnsxsnniosumlnssrslaflsnsnins (pp 360-396). San Francisco: Freeman. 96 Roy, M. (1977). A current survey of 150 cases. In M. Roy (Ed.),B_31131311mmg_n. (pp. 25-44). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Sigelman, C. K., Berry,C. J., 8W1les, K. A. (1984). Violence in college students' dating relationships WW. 6 530-548 Steinmetz, S. K (1977). Wife-beating, husband-beating- a comparison of the use of physical violence between spouses to resolve marital fights. In M. Roy (Ed.), W311 (pp. 63-71). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Steinmetz, S. K. (1980). Women and violence: Victims and perpetrators. AmsdsaMnumaLnLEsxsbstnsLacx. 3.4. 334-350 Stets, J. 8 Pirog-Good, M. (1987). Wolence in dating relationships. 53313) EsxsnnlscLQuansle. 60. 237-246. Straus, M. A. (1974) Leveltng, CIVIIIty and violence in the family mm W. 36. 13-29. Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The conflict tactics (CT) scales JoumaLnLManiamndesEamilx. 41. 75-88 Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., 8Steinmetz, S. K. (198mm W. New York: Doubleday/Anchor. Walker, L. E. (1979). W. New York: Harper and Row. Walker, L E. (1984). W. New York: Springer. Yllo, K. 8 Straus, M. A. (1981). Interpersonal violence among cohabitating and married couples- Eamilxfislatisns. 36. 339-348.