l [y 9 l/fljl/llllfllI/W MW ”8m 9'014 Michifl‘m 5“” / M W , , »/ / University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Transference Manifestations in Psychotherapy presented by Wendy Sabbath has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Psychology Major professor Date 12/3/86 MS U i: an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 MSU LIBRARIES m V RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. TRANSFERENCE MANIFESTATIONS IN PSYCHOTHERAPY By ‘Wendy June Sabbath A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1987 ABSTRACT TRANSFERENCE MANIFESTNTIONS IN PSYCHOTHERAPY By wendy June Sabbath An initial consensus seems to have been reached across practitioners of the major forms of psychotherapy regarding the importance of the relationship between therapist and patient, as a prominent factor in the process and eventual outcome of the therapy. The phenomena of transference, or the re-creation within the relationship with the therapist of earlier object relationships, has always been the cornerstone of psychoanalytic theory and technique. Although there has been lively interest within the psychoanalytic field in the vicissitudes of the transference and perceptions of the therapist, there have been relatively few empirical studies of . transference itself. The present study investigated the extent to which the therapist became perceived as similar to the mother or the father in the course of psychodynamic psychotherapy. Sixteen female clients undergoing six to ten months of psychotherapy at the M.S.U. Counseling Center served as subjects. The Rorschach was administered at both the beginning and the end of therapy, the Interpersonal Check List (ICIJ was administered at beginning, middle, and end of therapy, and the Helping Relationship Questionnaire (HRQ) was given at the end of therapy. Internal representations of the self and the mother changed significantly (p _<_ .05) from early to mid-therapy. 'Ihe self- representation also changed significantly from mid- to late therapy. Adaptive psychological functioning was significantly correlated with perceiving the therapist as similar to both mother (2 = .004) and father (2 = .02) in mid-therapy. 'Ihe therapist seemed to become perceived as more similar to mother or father in mid-therapy, and again less similar to mother or father in late therapy, but not t0>a significant extent. The total amount of change in representations of self, therapist, and mother was significantly QEHS .05) correlated with the amount of change in anxiety content on the Rorschach. Tbtal change in representations of therapist, mother, and father was significantly (p _<_ .05) correlated with the amount of change in hostility content on the Rorschach. No increase was found in capacity for object relations, as measured by the Developmental Analysis of the Concept of the Object scale applied to Rorschach responses. Intensity of late therapy representations was not significantly correlated with late therapy mean object relations scores. AWS It's very difficult to do justice to the profound contributions made by each member of my dissertation and guidance comnittees. Each of these people has been an integral and deeply valued part of my life for the past six years. Any kind of recognition must begin with Dr. Bert Karon. From the first lectures of his I heard in 1975 he has catalyzed my interest in personality and psychoanalytic psychology. Over the past years, he has been a staunch friend, advisor, and an inspiring supervisor. I appreciate both the freedom he gave me to develop my own ideas, and the careful attention and consideration he gave to them. Dr. Rabin gave generously of his wisdom and concern. Working with him for the past several years has been an honor, as well as a pleasure. He has introduced me to numerous aspects of psychoanalytic thought, and his interest has enriched my life in more personal ways as well. Dr. Abeles was a supportive, informative, and very helpful supervisor and committee member. Dr. Powell was cheerfully encouraging, both in terns of research and in clinical issues. I would like to thank the staff of the M.S.U. Counseling Center for their cooperation with this project. I appreciate the attempts of all the therapists who solicited the participation of their clients. I'd like to thank the subjects themselves, who gave their time and were willing to share their personal experiences and feelings with me. iv Richard was a dedicated and humorous tester and scorer. Carol, as always, was a unique deeply appreciated consultant, test, and ally. Suzy's technical and adndnistrative assistance has been consistently over and above what was required, and is also appreciated. TABLE OF CONTENTS LI ST OF TABLES 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O CHAPTERl IWMICN. O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Transference................... Einpirical Studies of Transference. . . . . . . . . Developmental Aspects of Internal Representations. Summary . Theoretical Approaches to Object Relations and Recent Applications of Projective Interpersonal Theories and Research on Stages of Psychotherapy. . . . . . . . Summary of Literature. . . . CHAPTER 2 MISC O O O O O O O O O O 0 CHAPTER 3 ms 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O Sllbjxt O O O O O I O 0 Subjects and Therapist. Premature Terminators . Instruments . . . . . . Prm$ureSO O O O O O O Scoring Procedures. . . CHAPTER 4 RESULTS . Main Hypotheses. . . . . . . CHAPTERS Techniques . . Psychotherapy Rorschach............. Helping Relationship Questionnaire. DI$USSIWQ O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 vi Page viii 11 15 15 21 23 25 28 33 33 33 35 36 40 42 47 47 53 55 6O @mral Issues 0 O O O O I O O O O O C O O O O O O O The Relationship between Change in Representations and Change in Rorschach Content. . . . . . . . Hostility, Anxiety, and Level of Functioning. . Change in the Capacity for Object Relations . . Quality of the Transference . . . . . . . . . . Maternal/Paternal Aspects and Temporal Changes Later Therapy ICL and Object Relations Scores APPEND ICES APPENDIX A. APPENDIX B. APPENDIX C . APPENDIX D. ms. in Transference. . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii 60 65 66 67 68 71 72 73 75 76 77 78 LIST OF TABLES Tables 1 T-tests of Differences between Prematurely Terminating Clients and Clients Completing Therapy on Initial variables. 0 O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Rorschach Scores Used as Criteria for Level of PersonalityFunctioning................. Mean ICL Representation Scores for Self, Therapist, Mother, and Father in Early, Middle, and Late Therapy. . Mean Early and Late-therapy Rorschach HoStility, Anxiety, Object Representation, and Criteria Scores. . . Pearson Correlations between Total Amount of Change in ICL Representations of Self, Therapist, Mother and Father and Total Change from Early to Late Rorschach Hostility, Anxiety, Mean Object Representation and CriteriaScores..................... T-tests Comparing Rorschach Object Representation Scores in Early and in Late Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Rorschach variables between First and Second Administrations . . . . . . . . Descriptive Statistics, Frequencies and Percentages for RorSChaCh variable 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Descriptive Statistics for the Helping Relationship meStiomBir-e O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 viii ‘ Page 37 43 48 51 51 52 54 56 58 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION The foundation of the psychotherapy literature has traditionally rested on individual case histories and on clinicians' reports of therapy's efficacy, but many of the factors influencing therapy process and outcome have proved difficult to isolate and to quantify. Achieving understanding of the many influences operating in this process has been, according to Greenspan and Wieder (1984), one of the most difficult challenges in the behavioral sciences. Graff and Luborsky (I977) highlighted one difficult problem in quantitative analysis of therapeutic process: discovering meaningful long-term trends in therapy by means of both economical and reasonable methods. Their hope was that solutions to this problem might provide verification of clinical findings. It is one thing to assert, as Smith, Glass, and Miller (1980) did, that psychotherapy is consistently beneficial and that ”its efficacy has been demonstrated with near monotonous regularity” (p. 183), but the question which remains, and to Which this study will attempt to supply some answers, is just how events in psychotherapy, and specifically aspects of the therapeutic relationship and the transference itself, interact to produce successful outcome in psychotherapy. Some previous studies of the relationship between therapist and client have offered observations about important features of this 1 relationship from both the therapists' and the clients' points of view. Leary and Harvey (1956) described psychotherapy as an "inplausible procedure offering to the individual the opportunity to learn those things about himself which by definition he does not*wish.to know” (p. 123). In the face of such opposition, they felt that the continued existence of psychotherapy was in itself sufficient testimony to its effectiveness, a position reminiscent of Freud's (1937) frequently cited definition of analysis as one of three ”impossible professions in which one can be sure beforehand of achieving unsatisfying results" (p. 248). Janet Malcolm (1981) followed this observation with Limentani's crunent that psychoanalysis ”is not only impossible, but also extremely difficult" (Malcolm, 1981). Iapkin and Lury (1983) took a developmental perspective toward process, and suggested that psychotherapy should focus on repairing previous omissions in the developmental building blocks of the individual's life such as identifications, object constancy, and reciprocity. Again, the emphasis was on the reparative possibilities inherent in the relationship between therapist and client, and in the ways in which this relationship was handled. They referred to Guntrip's statement: Psychotherapy involves that the patient must gr w out of unrealistic positive and negative transference relations, in which he is seeing his internal fantasied good and bad objects projected in to his therapist, by means of discovering what kind of actual relationship is given to him by his therapist as a real person. This involves much more than experienced psychoanalytical interpretation. That paves the way, against the background of the kind of person the analyst actually is, for the patient to grow'gradually to an accurate perception of him as a real person in his own right. (Guntrip, 1973, p. 66) Lapkin and Lury concurred that this perspective toward the therapeutic relationship transcended classical psychoanalytic technique and constituted a 'personal relationship therapy' (Guntrip, 1973, p. 183) which included all the variants of psychodynamically oriented psychotherapy. Stmpp (Goldfried, 1980) believed: Psychotherapy works because (a) all human beings have a strong tendency to 'transfer' patterns of interpersonal relatedness from the past to the present, and (b) this transference tendency can be utilized by the therapist to bring about a 'corrective experience.‘ Thus the essence of psychotherapy is interpersonal learning, a new significant experience (ERLEENIS) that, if all goes well, modifies basic aspects of the patient's patterns of relatedness in ways that are called therapeutic....By patterns of interpersonal relatedness I mean characteristic and often stereotyped ways in which a person relates to significant others. Such patterns become apparent particularly in situations that are affectively charged and that activate engrained (overlearned) tendencies of relating to care-givers and authority figures. Patterns of relatedness are often sufosed by unconscious fantasies and contradictory strivings. (p. 279) Definitions such as the preceding ones provide for the emergence of transference reactions in all forms of psychotherapy, but only in psychodynamically oriented psychotherapy and psychoanalysis are transference vicissitudes both anticipated and directly addressed as crucial aspects of the treatment. The existing literature on transference has been oriented toward theoretical speculations regarding transference and clinical vignettes, but in recent years there has been renewed interest in the therapeutic relationship, and therapeutic alliance in general, as well as specific qualities of the transference. Transference ‘Waelder (1956) briefly defined transference as "an attempt of the patient to revive and re-enact, in the analytic situation and in relation to the analyst, situations and fantasies of his childhood" (p. 367). waelder mentioned bringing influence fer change to bear upon the client in the form of new identifications in the superego or the ego, and of therapy functioning as a corrective experience. He enumerated three general approaches to the transference: (1) transference as an obstacle to the treatment, (2) transference as possibly THE most important vehicle of the treatment, and (3) transference as the opportunity of direct influence, through identification or re-training. Zetzel (1956) wrote of the development of traditional views of transference moving from the structural approach to include greater consideration of current, past, internal, and external object relations and their effect both on the patient's phenomenological world and the therapeutic relationship. She extensively discussed differing psychoanalytic perspectives on transference, noting that crucial aspects of the early relationship, and their repetition in the analytic situation, were consensually validated events occurring with great frequency. Schlesingerévaccaro (1983) drew explicit parallels between transference phenomena and Piaget's concept of conservation. She pointed out that mental representations of persons and of situations were conserved, and in the transference the patient revealed the inner world as it had been conserved. The goal of therapy was seen as aiding the patient to attempt to assimilate the therapist less to mother- and father-schemata, and to work toward increased acconmodation with the external environment. Gill (1982) reiterated that transference clearly occurred in therapy and ought to be taken seriously in all 'uncovering' modes of therapy. He distinguished between two types of transference interpretation: interpretations of resistance to the awareness of transference, and interpretations of resistance to the resolution of transference. Two types of resistance to resolution of transference were enumerated. The first.were interpretations working with the transference in the present by pointing out that the client's attitude might not be as clearly determined by the in'nediate therapeutic situation as he or she may claim. The second were the 'genetic transference interpretations,‘ or interpretations of a similarity between the transference attitude and the past. Konig and Tischtau-Schroter's (1982) definition of transference explicitly mentioned both its object-relations and interpersonal aspects: Transference is defined as a projection of inner objects and parts of the self; the classical part of transference always contains a nucleus of projection. The interactional part of transference consists in verbal and nonverbal behavior with the purpose of causing the other person to comply with the transferred object or part of self: Transference expectations may be unconscious and in conflict*with conscious expectations. (p. 1191) Schafer (1977) clarified the relationship between relived experience and new experience in the interpersonal situation of the transference. He noted that the positive aspects of the transference ('transference love') related to both the current life situation and to the repressed past. A sequence of transference reactions was suggested in which the analyst's consistent containing and tolerating of aggressive impulses and feelings was seen as a pre-condition for development of the client's capacity for loving. Having reached this phase, the experience of having both positive and negative reactions toward therapist was hypothesized to lead to both less splitting and less idealization of the therapist, and to culminate in the patient's increased ability to differentiate from both parents and analyst. The analysand now tolerates his or her own experiencing of relationships ambivalently, thereby demonstrating that new and more inclusive conditions for loving have been developed. This development bears as much on loving oneself as on loving others. That is to say, tolerating the ambivalent mode is, correlatively, a condition of healthy 'narcissism' and 'object love': the one implies the other rather than being at its expense. (PP. 344-345) Karme' (1982) proposed that the predominantly maternal or paternal quality of the transference was predicted by the therapist's gender and developmental qualities of both the client and the current depth of the analysis itself. She suggested that maternal transferences occurred toward male and female analysts, but existed only in preoedipal phases for patients with male therapists. Similarly, she believed that a true paternal transference neurosis to female analysts were quite rare, and that during analysis of the oedipal phase transference became specific to the gender of the analyst. Mogul (1982) extensively reviewed the existing literature on the effect of the therapist's sex on therapy. She found agreement among a number of clinical reports that gender of the psychoanalyst had been related to the sequence in which transference reactions occurred, and that this effect had been most evident early in treatment. Her conclusions were that the specific content and sequence of transference reactions were related to therapist gender in neurotic patients, that more developmentally regressed and fragmentary transference reactions, which were affected by therapist gender, were more dramatic in developmentally regressed patients, and that therapist gender had less impact in classical psychoanalysis with.neurotic patients than in other forms of psychotherapy with neurotic and other patient populations. Kulish's 1984 literature review confirmed these conclusions. Kulish specifically commented upon the absence of a body of research to support the significant number of clinical observations by such notable figures as Freud, Glover, and Greenson, and that the phase of PE treatment, developmental fixation and maturation of the patient, and therapist gender were all involved in the specific maternal or paternal quality of the transference at a given moment in therapy. «.1 Empirical Studies of Transference Luborsky, Crabtree, Curtis, Ruff, and Mintz (1975) attempted to make a 'conceptual map' of dimensions of transference based on a factor analysis of eight psychoanalysts' ratings of 23 clinically relevant transference concepts from transcriptions of segments of a successful analysis. The original definition of transference which guided the study was influenced by object-relations theory: ”Transference is a revival in a current object relationship, especially to the analyst, of thought, feeling, and behavior derived from repressed fantasies originating in significant conflictual childhood relationships” (p. 65). Four basic dimensions were found, the largest factor (accounting for approximately one fifth of the variance) being 'expressed parental transference.’ The other three factors were 'infantile distortion' (material seen by the analyst as infantile and distorted), 'positive transference,’ and 'sibling transference.‘ All transference manifestations decreased over the course of the analysis, confirming theoretical beliefs that successful outcome is marked by, and/or 8 somewhat the result of, lessening of rigid pathological patterns of relating to others. Mueller (1969b) conceptualized transference much along the lines of Gill's 'genetic transference interpretations,‘ observing: The pattern of a client's reactions to his therapist often recapitulates the behavioral patterns that the client learned as modes of coping with stress in earlier parent-child interactions and that those repetitious behavioral patterns are often indices of generic conflicts. (p. 2) Mueller investigated the ways in which the pattern of client reactions to the therapist converged throughout therapy with the client's previous reactions to parents and other significant persons. He found that, particularly when anxious, the client tended to behave toward the therapist as he or she had reported behavioring toward the parents. The therapist then tended to behave toward the client in a similar fashion to the parents' reported behaviors. meller interpreted these reciprocal interactions as clear evidence for the development of transference relationships during therapy, and for occurrence of predictable countertransference reactions. Hoornaert and Pierloot (1976) referred to symbolic dimensions of parental figures as "affective and mental schemata, formed by the personal relation with the parents, enriched by the significant additions awarded by the cultural world of the subject” (p. 261). Their primary interest.was in the maternal and paternal aspects of transference manifested in the attitudes and feelings of psychosomatic patients toward their physicians. A.unique aspect of this study was its documentation of the occurrence of transference in a setting analogous, but not identical, to psychotherapy. The patient's image of the doctor was found to have a predominance of paternal (autonomy, dynamism, and action) qualities, and the correlation was higher between images of the doctor and tie ideal fatter than that between the doctor and actual fatter. The representation of the doctor showed almst no similarity to eitrer the image of tie real or the ideal motler. Blotcky (1984) suggested that an early examination of human content and human movement responses in Rorschach protocols might anticipate transference distortions which would emerge later in psychotherapy. He described tie rapid development of a turbulent transference, often including both devaluation and idealization, as characteristic features of psychoanalytic psychotherapy with borderline adolescents. He hoped that accurate anticipation of such transference patterns might help prevent pranature termination, acting-out, development of a therapeutic inpasse, or countertherapeutic behaviors on tre part of the trerapist. ‘mo relatively recent studies have reported attempts to both describe and treasure changes in the transference across the course of psychoanalysis and psychotherapy. Although the investigators conceptualized the role of the transference in slightly different ways, the emergence of such studies highlights the growing trend in psychodynamic research of recognizing the importance of tie therapeutic relationship and of transference. Graff and Luborsky (1977) charted analysts' quantitative ratings of transference and resistance for each session of four patients' analyses in order to trace the analytic process in the dynamic unfolding of transference and resistance. They naintained a specifically object-relations conceptualization of transference : 10 The degree to which the patient is dealing with material that is overtly or covertly related to the analyst. This material would be a manifestation of or a displacement from an early inrxutant object relation. The previous object, however, does not have to be mentioned; it may be inferred by the rater because of inappropriateness, etc. (p. 476) Distinct patterns of clinical change were found for the four patients. The two whose posttherapeutic outcome was considered more successful showed increasing levels of transference concomitant with decreasing levels of resistance. comparing thiS‘with the‘widely held belief that successful outcome is expected to be related to decreasing resistance, the authors suggested that the pattern of change associated with successful psychotherapy was movement from unconscious, unrecognized, transference manifestations in affect and behavior to a more conscious, and therefore more manageable, transference reaction. This seems to echo, in quantitative fonm, Freud's original rule of psychoanalysis 'to make the unconscious conscious'. Graff and luborsky's opinion was that the patients were no more disturbed, but that the transference theme remained evident in a context of increased mastery. The two less successful cases showed fairly parallel transference and resistance curves without the decrease in resistance characteristic of the more successful analyses, suggesting that the transference reactions could not be worked through as resistance to analytic work in general renained so high. Rawn (1981) made several comments on this study. First, he cited his own 1958 paper as being the pioneer attempt to quantitatively examine transference and resistance variables. More important, he noted that his own original results were identical‘with those of Graff and Luborsky. In both studies, the presence of heightened resistance 11 coupled with negative transference was inevitably found to lead to unsuccessful therapeutic outcome. Gill and Hoffman (1982) had judges code transcripts of audio- recorded psychotherapy sessions for evidence of 'experience of the (therapeutic) relationship', or transference manifestations and the ways in which they were handled in therapy. These authors differed somewhat from Graff and Luborsky in their explicit.concern‘with both preconscious aspects of transference as well as the actual behavior of the therapist expected to impact on the transference. This attention to the significance of tie trerapists' actual statements and behaviors brings this study conceptually close to a number of interpersonally- oriented investigations (Crowder, 1972; Dietzel & Abeles, 1975: Mueller, 1969a, b; Mueller & Dilling, 1968). It is because of transference that a patient is sensitive to certain facets of the 'reality' of the therapist as opposed to others which he may ignore....The transference may manifest itself in selective attention to some particular aspect of the complex reality of the analyst's behavior, or in some fixed mode of adaptation to that response. But it need not and, indeed, usually does not distort reality in any simple sense. (Gill & Hoffman, 1982, p. 14) In this report of their ongoing work, Gill and Heffman claimed impressive reliability data for ratings of transference and expressed their joint certainty that a real clinical phenomenon, with crucial implications for future research, had been measured. Developmental Aspects of Internal Representations Several studies report changes in identification patterns occurring with successful psychotherapy. Groesbeck (1978) considered this issue from a Jungian perspective, believing that the natural 12 evolution of interpretations of psychological types in the analysis of the transference was related to later individuation. In support, he cited Paulsen, "Because it transcends them, the term transference can legitimately be distinguished from the term projection and used to designate the successive stages of the individuation process as it occurs in relation to the analyst" (Groesbeck, 1978, p. 44). In clinical settings, more disturbed clinical populations have been found to display identifications characteristic of earlier stages of psychosocial development (Cava & Rausch, 1952), and quantitatively less intense (Sopchak, 1952) than those of nonsymptomatic individuals. Cava and Rausch observed conflict in areas related to identification in psychoanalytic theory to be predictably related to twelfthrgrade boys' perceptions of their fathers. Less conflicted boys tended to identify more strongly with their fathers, and significant (p < .10) differences discriminated between more— and less-conflicted boys in castration anxiety, total identification, Oedipal intensity, and identification differences. Sopchak asked 108 students to complete MMPIs for themselves, and as they thought their father, their mother, and 'most people' would respond. In this nonclinical sample, identification with parents was correlated with good judgement, while abnormality (a configuration of normatively deviant MMPI scores) correlated with lack of identification with either parent or with people in general. Better-adjusted male subjects tended to identify more strongly with fathers than with mothers, but for women both positive identification *with the mother and failure to identify with the father were positively correlated with abnormality. Another MMPI study with a nonclinical population (Beier & Ratzenburg, 1953) found significant gender l3 differences in identifications with mother and father, with each gender identifying more readily with.the parent of the same sex. McDonald (1965) studied hostility and identifications in a sample of single white pregnant.women in the seventh month of pregnancy. MMPI scores were used as indexes of ego control patterns and the Interpersonal Check List (ICIJ was used to measure representations of the self, mother, and father. Significant differences were found between members of four ego-control groups (expressors, repressors, sensitizers, or expressor-sensitizers), and attribution of hostility to parents was related to large differences between self-representation and representations of parents: the less the subject identified with a given parent, the more hostility was attributed to that parent. Krieger and Wbrchel (1960) investigated patterns of parental identifications in a "neurotic' university clinical population, a homosexual clinical population, and a "normal" heterosexual non- clinical pOpulation using a Orsort.technique to elicit representations of the self, the ideal self, the mother, and the father. Normals identified nearly equally with both parents, and neurotics tended to identify significantly more with the opposite-sexed parent. Male neurotics' ideal self was significantly more highly correlated with mother than with father, while female neurotics' ideal self was more like the father than the mother. Hbmosexual males' ideal selves correlated more highly‘with the father than with the mother. Mueller (1966) reported correlations between university students' semantic differential ratings for parents and self and anxiety scores from the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale. Highly anxious male subjects described themselves and their parents as more passive, impotent, and 14 poorly evaluated than the low anxious group. Low anxious males regarded their fathers significantly more highly than themselves, while the highly anxious males regarded their fathers as much like themselves. High anxious females attributed more passivity to their fathers than did the low anxious females. Mueller speculated that while low anxious individuals seemed to have discrete representations of the father and themselves, more anxious individuals' self- representations were patterned after the denigrated father. less anxious females particularly depicted a psychological sense of distance or differentiation between themselves and their fathers. Results also suggested that females' responses carried better validity in studies of psychotherapy than males' as females seemed less affected by situational variables such as anxiety. Schrier (1953) investigated the relationship between outcome of short-term psychotherapy and aspects of the therapeutic relationship, and in particular the patient's identification with the therapist. He cited Fuchs, ”Each identification is therefore a monument of an object relation, and our character, being itself composed of identifications, contains the history of our object relations" (Schrier, 1953, p. 586). Schrier felt this was similar to Freud's belief that identification, an elementary capacity to form an emotional relationship, was a function of the capacity for object relationships. His study of nine hospitalized short-term psychotherapy patients yielded highly significant (p < .001) relationships between variables measuring identification, a positive therapeutic relationship, and positive outcome. 15 weiner (1982) discussed theoretical issues relevant to identification in psychotherapy. Beginning with Freud's concept that identification might be the earliest expression.of an emotional relationship, he suggested that growing identification with highly valued persons might be one of the most important processes taking place in psychotherapy, concluding that some degree of identification ‘with the therapist was necessary for eventual individuation in psychological development. MEX The preceding studies indicate the importance of internal representations of others. Such representations have been shown to highlight pathological modes of regarding others, and change as a result of successful psychotherapy. Given the assumption that identification is the one of the earliest forms of an object relationship, another group of studies involving object relations, a number of which utilize the Rorschach, have relevance here in terms of placing differentiation as a developmental process naturally succeeding identification, and therefore representing movement towards integration of internal representations and psychological development. Theoretical Approaches to Object Relations and Recent Applications of Projective Techniques Recent developments (Blatt & Lerner, 1983) in psychoanalytic theory have stressed a less ”mechanistic" and increasingly interactive ViGW'Of personality development, in which "ongoing object relations are internalized as part of the development of processes of representation and thereby become part of the personality structure" (p. 8). The 16 process of internalizing significant interpersonal relationships is believed to lead to the construction of cognitive-affective schemes which thereafter govern the individual's construction of the world and, most important, representations of the self in interaction with others. In cases where these existing psychological constructions have interfered with.adjustment, the therapeutic relationship has appeared to be the natural vehicle for the development of more adaptive representations of self and others. If the internalization of object relations results in the formation of psychic structures during normal development, then the internalization of significant interactions between the patient and the therapist must play an important role in the therapeutic process. The therapist becomes available as a new object by eliminating, step by step, the transference distortions which interfere with the establishment of new object relationships (Loewald, 1960). And it is the internalization of new and relatively undistorted relations ‘with the therapist which leads to therapeutic change. But the clinical examples presented in this paper suggest that there are different types of distortions in the context and structure of the representational world in various forms of psychopathology. These differences should be expressed in the therapeutic process in the nature of the transference and countertransference. The quality of the object representations prior to beginning therapy should provide understanding about the potential transference issues that may emerge in therapy. It should also provide information about the types of changes that need to occur in the individual's representational world if the patient is to progress toward higher developmental levels. Different types of changes may occur in different types of patients depending upon the disruptions of their representational world when they begin treatment. (p. 26) Lerner (1983) wrote, "The development of representations is regarded as the sine qua non of ego development and adaptation" (p. 314). He mentioned the growing interest in studies of interpersonal processes relating to internalized representations and anticipated transference paradigms to counter criticisms of the ego 17 psychological model as being conceptually distant from the experiential world. Lerner, among others, applied the Blatt, Brennais, SChimek, and Glick (1976) scale to Rorschach protocols collected fromlclinical populations. The scale was based on wernerian principles and was designed for assessment of the concept of the object on the Rorschach. In the original article, Blatt et al. reported that formal properties of human responses on the Rorschach showed consistent changes with the developmental level and degree of psychopathology of the patient. Developmentally advanced responses,‘with higher levels of differentiation, integration, and articulation, were found to occur with increased psychological development, and to diminish in direct proportion to psychological disturbance. Spear and Lapidus (1981) found significant differences between groups of obsessive/paranoid borderline, hysterical/impulsive borderline, and undifferentiated schizophrenic patients in their quality of object relations as measured by the Blatt et a1. scale. Schwager and Spear (1981) compared pre- and post-therapy protocols of ten chronic schizophrenic patients who had been treated in a long-term psychiatric inpatient unit. Different directional shifts were found in change criteria for paranoid and for nonparanoid undifferentiated patients. The paranoid group showed a significant decrease in overall levels of cognitive-structural differentiation while the nonparanoid group showed the expected significant increase in capacity for object differentiation. Lerner (1983) used this scale to illustrate clinical change following intensive psychoanalytic psychotherapy of a sixteen- year—old girl and demonstrated dramatic changes in her human representations on the Rorschach which were consistent‘with progressive 18 transference manifestations. He attributed these changes to the effectiveness of therapy in aiding the patient to integrate part-object relations into whole object relations, promoting object constancy, and achieving an integrated self-concept. Ryan and Bell (1984) investigated changes in representations of self and others over an extended period of recovery fromlpsychosis in order to further test assumptions of object-relations views of psychopathology. They used a scale which assessed object relations through the patient's early memories, measuring concepts similar to those of Blatt et a1. (1976). Level of object representation was found to increase significantly from tie psychotic level at initial hospitalization to the level at postrtreatment followup. PatientS'with low object relations scores at followup were likely to be rehospitalized at a rate almost twice as high as that of individuals whose followup object relations scores were greater, supporting the hypothesis that object relations scores were indicators of psychopathology. Tuber (1983) used both Rorschach structural and object relations variables to predict later adjustment in children treated at a residential facility. He found object relations scores to be significantly related to incidence of hospitalization at followup, with highest scorers most successful at avoiding later institutionalization. Tuber suggested that this constitutes evidence that "these individual's' early internalized representations of others were benign enough to enhance the likelihood of generating equally benign relationships later in life" (p. 383). l9 Lerner and St. Peter (1984) looked at developmental patterns of Object relations across a wide range of psychopathology, comparing Rorschach percepts scored according to the Blatt et al. scale from groups of patients falling within neurotic, borderline, and schizophrenic categories. Further supporting the developmental basis of this concept, they found "a steadily increasing relationship between a person's quality of reality testing (defined by response accuracy), developmental level of the concept of the object (developmental index) and psychopathology" (p. 88). As predicted, the most impaired patients presented the most poorly differentiated, articulated, and integrated figures and the least impaired patients presented the most well perceived responses. Several underlying concepts link the above studies. First, it seems that processes of identification and differentiation are crucial to the development in capacity for object relationships, and may be demonstrated in the resultant quality of object representations. The level of development of these representations varies directlylwith level of psychopathology and has been reliably assessed by means of projective techniques, such as the Rorschach, The Blatt et al. scale appears to be the most widely used, but similar scales (Krohn & Mayman, 1974; Urist, 1977; Urist & Shill, 1982) have been used‘with.success as well. Athey, Fleischer, and Coyne (1980) accounted for the popularity of the Blatt et al. scale by the fact that "it is the only empirical attempt to address the relationship between thought organization and object relations in any systematic sense, illustrating some degree of correspondence between group thought disorder and developmental qualities of object representation" (p. 276-277). Second, these 20 otherwise relatively hidden internal representations become apparent when reenacted in the relationship with the therapist, both in interpersonal behavior and in the perceptions and distortions of the transference itself, and become demonstrably more developmentally advanced at the conclusion of successful psychotherapy. Classical psychoanalysis, the ego—psychology/object relations perspective, and the interpersonal perspective have all made contributions in trese areas, reaching remarkably similar conclusions. Freud himself directly addressed the existence of both the reality- based interpersonal relationship and transference proper in his 1937 discussion of "a certain man" and the man's analyst. Ernest Jpnes later identified the analyst and analysand as Freud himself and Ferenczi. Freud discussed the advantages and disadvantages of interpreting the negative transference: To activate it would certainly have required some unfriendly piece of behavior in reality on the analyst's part. Furthermore, he (the analyst) added, not every good relation between an analyst and his subject during and after analysis was to be regarded as a transference; there were also friendly relations which were based on reality and which proved to be viable. (p. 222) Along similar lines, Gill (1982) wrote: It may be that the recognition that all resistance interpretations are interpersonal interpretations has been obscured by tie fear that to do so would comnit oneself to an interpersonal rather than an intrapsychic View of human psychology. But one need not be driven into such a dichotomy....the ultimate outcome of the interaction of givens and environmental factors is the intrapsychic formation which becomes interpersonally expressed in human interaction. (p. 41) 21 Interpersonal Theories and Research on Psychotherapy A.number of studies investigating clients' internal representations of self and others, and changes in such representations subsequent to therapy, have used the Interpersonal Check List (ICL). Clark and Taulbee (1981) felt that although the ICL had been widely used in the evaluation of psychotherapy outcome, its potential for assessment of interpersonal perceptions and behavior had not been tapped. Leary and Harvey (1956) used the ICL to study direction of personality change in psychotherapy along the two major ICL parameters of dominance-submission and hostility-affiliation. Men who had rated themselves as 'hostile' or 'weak' before treatment were significantly more likely to change than were women who rated themselves the same way. HOwever, men who were 'conventional' and 'bland' before therapy had a poorer prognosis than women rated that.way. Mueller and Dilling (1968) scored the actual interactions of nineteen therapist-client pairs according to ICL categories in an investigation of reciprocal effects of therapist-client interactions, those "attempts on the part of each person to establish an emotional state in the interaction which tends to elicit a predictable response from the other person" (p. 282). Ample evidence for the existence of such interactions was found, particularly in competitive—hostile therapist behavior which.was significantly correlated with competitive- hostile client behavior as well as subtle passive-resistant behavior by the client. Supportive-interpretative therapist behavior was positively correlated with support-seeking client behavior. A second paper (Mueller & Dilling, 1969) proposed using the same interpersonal system to measure similarities between the client's interaction‘with 22 the therapist and his or her descriptions of other interpersonal relationships, tracking interpersonal themes and observing in.what.ways mention of such themes paralleled, preceded, or followed client- therapist interactions. The authors stated that this concept was basically an operational definition of transference and that "this weaving together of past and present interactions, constantly using one to explain the other, is the way that a therapeutic relationship develops; so our system‘would seem to have clinical validity" (p. 51). Lorr, McNair, Michaux, and Raskin (1962) used four subscales of groupings of ICL items, and several other personality and symptom measures to study the effect of the frequency of psychotherapy on therapeutic outcome. ICL scores documented improvement after eight months of treatment. One year after initiation of psychotherapy, reduction in severity of distress was associated with general gains in interpersonal relations as measured on the ICL. The pattern of changes suggested to the authors that successful psychotherapy followed a natural progression in which symptoms were initially reduced, followed by subtler gains in the capacity for interpersonal relations. Kiesler (1982) felt that interpersonal theorists had paid insufficient attention of psychotherapy issues, and extensively reviewed the various interpersonal measures with particular emphasis to the concept of complementarity. He suggested that since conplementarity stabilized and restricted the range of interpersonal behavior, therapists could produce maximal change by providing acomplementary responses to clients, thereby increasing the client's repertoire of interpersonal experiences. He warned that the strong anxiety engendered by consistently responding in an acomplementary 23 fashion might strain the limits of a fragile therapeutic alliance, and suggested a two-stage approach to psychotherapy. In the first, 'hooked,‘ stage, the therapist would experience being virtually unable to respond except in a complenentary fashion to a client who had many years of experience in interpersonal manuevers designed (unconsciously) to elicit just such responses in others. The second, 'disengagement', phase would be marked by the therapist's gradual regaining of the ability to respond freely in both.complementary and acomplementary ways. Stages of Psychotherapy Several other theorists have proposed that successful psychotherapy progresses in fairly discrete, predictable stages. Dietzel and Abeles (1975) found a three-stage pattern of complementary behaviors in their study of twenty psychotherapy cases. Successful and unsuccessful therapist-client dyads were marked by high levels of complementarity in the initial stages of therapy. More disturbed clients were skilled at eliciting more complementarity at this stage, but this was not related to eventual therapy outcome. In the middle stage of therapy, successful dyads showed significantly lower levels of complementarity than unsuccessful ones, indicating that "to facilitate client change, the therapist must avoid a confirming, security- enhancing stance to client elicitations, and therapists who become caught up in, or ensnared by, the client's constricted elicitations will reinforce these maladjusted behavioral modes" (p. 271). Successful dyads continued to exhibit less complementarity in the late, or termination, stage of therapy. 24 Crowder (1972) looked at the relationship between interpersonally scored transference and countertransference in therapist-client dyads and psychotherapy outcome in early, middle, and late stages of therapy. countertransference in early interviews discriminated between successful and unsuccessful dyads. Clients who improved were more support-seeking in early and middle, but not in late, therapy. They tended to be significantly more hostile-competitive and less passive- resistant and supportive-interpretive early in therapy. In mid- therapy, nonimproving clients continued to express more passive- resistant behaviors. Crowder concluded that the quality of interpersonally expressed transference and countertransference reactions were indicative of the outcome of psychotherapy. Successful dyads seemed to have established an alliance in which the client.was willing to flexibly experiment with therapist-initiated new interpersonal actions, while unsuccessful dyads appeared to become bogged down in manuevers which maintained the status quo. Tracey and Ray (1984) found that therapy moved progressively through three stages of complementarity. The first stage continued until an initial high level of complementarity was reached. Upon reaching this point, the middle stage, marked by a low level of complementarity, began. At this point, therapist and client appeared to struggle to both maintain an alliance and to work through persistent conflicts. In the last stage, complementarity reached a higher level, indicating ”a new, more realistic consensus between the participants as to what form the relationship should take" (p. 25). Greenspan and‘Wieder (1984) described the stages of the therapeutic relationship as: 25 Becoming invested in an emotional sense in the person of the therapist (as well as the program of therapy), engaging in an exchange of signals that are purposeful and organized (as a minimum basis for embarking on the techniques generic to the particular therapy): learning to tolerate even minimum discomfort generic to the particular treatment and continuing at higher levels of capacity to observe various behavioral and emotional patterns in the context of a stable affective therapeutic relationship and then engaging in.various ways of altering trese patterns. (p. 6) Eimmmuapgifliterature The most immediate question regarding psychotherapy outcome research was once whether or not psychotherapy could be proven to have an effect which was demonstrably longer-lasting and more substantial than other forms of social reinforcement such as friendship, advice- giving, and benign attention. This question seems to have been answered in the affirmative with enough consistency to satisfy most critics. One major arena of current process and outcome studies has been the client's general capacity to form relationships and the qualities of such relationships, including elements of identifications and projections. Silverman and Weinberger (1985) believed that the activation of symbiotic fantasies accounted for much of the power of therapy to promote change. Clinical researchers have been especially interested in the influence of internal representations on interpersonal behavior and on the transference itself as therapy progresses, and in transfbrmations of representations and the quality of the transference throughout the course of therapy. Transference has frequently been conceptualized as the formation of an internal representation of the therapist, composed of elements of both fantasy and reality, the way in which the therapist is perceived as similar to significant persons in the client's early life, and the 26 tendency of the client to attempt to recreate old patterns of behavior with these significant persons in the therapeutic relationship. The two conditions marking the presence of transference and distinguishing it from other interpersonal behaviors are the repetitiveness and inappropriateness of the behavior (Crowder & Feltoon, 1973). crowder and Feltoon's extensive review of the literature yielded a number of studies documenting significant perceived similarity between childrens' perceptions of teachers and mothers, between 'the most important parental figure' and the therapist for group therapy participants, increasing similarity between parent and therapist with continuing psychotherapy, and inconsistent similarity between the sex of the therapist and that of the parent perceived as most similar to the therapist. Although it seems generally accepted that the transference may take on either a predominantly maternal or paternal quality, and that during long-term psychoanalysis oscillation between these two occurs, there is no consensus regarding immediate precipitants for the therapist becoming perceived as predominantly like mother or father, and little on the forms the quality of the transference takes in shorter-term psychodynamic psychotherapy. As most research on psychoanalytic concepts is carried out in psychoanalytic settings, there is a need to test these same concepts in the more prevalent setting of moderate-tenm psychodynamic or psychoanalytically-oriented psychotherapy. The present study will focus on two major aspects of the therapeutic relationship and therapy outcome: changes in internal representations of self and others throughout therapy with particular attention to the relationship of such changes to initial, final, and 27 total amounts of change in personality functioning, and the quality of the transference itself. Quality of the transference refers to progressive distortions in the perception of the therapist in ways which recapitulate earlier significant relationships, and the effect on these distortions of both initial level of client personality functioning and therapist gender. CHAPTER 2 The purpose of this study was to illustrate changes in the client's perception of the therapist across three stages of psychotherapy, and to link transfermations in internal representations of self, therapist, and parents with general growth in the capacity for object relationships and more adaptive personality functioning. l. Representations of the self, the therapist, the mother, and the father will change significantly from early therapy to mid-therapy and from mid-therapy to late therapy. This hypothesis was tested by scoring the ICL rating of self, therapist, mother, and father at each stage of therapy for dominance, affiliation and intensity (distance from the midpoint of the ICL circumplex). T-tests were then employed to determine whether a significant difference existed between ratings at consecutive phases of therapy. 2. In mid-therapy the representation.of the therapist will become significantly more similar to the representation of one parent than it had been in early therapy. The theoretical basis for this prediction was that because both client and therapist were aware of the time-limited nature of the therapy, the transference would take on a predominantly maternal or 28 29 paternal quality, resulting from the particular developmental conflict which the client was unconsciously re—enacting in the therapeutic relationship, stemming fromlthe client's level of psychosocial development. Although the transference is conceptualized as containing residues of relationships‘with both mother and father, which qualitatively shift from moment to moment, it was hypothesized that in this mode of therapy the therapist was perceived as predominantly like mother or like father, which was illustrated in shifts of ICL ratings of the therapist from the more objective perceptions of the early therapy stage to the more highly transferentially charged ratings of the middle therapy stage. Subjects were classified according to which parent the therapist was perceived as more similar in mid-therapy. Tbtests were used to determine whether the geometric distance between ICL scores for therapist and that parent lessened significantly from early therapy to mid-therapy. 3. Mid-therapy perception of the therapist as similar to one parent will be related to the client's initial level of personality functioning. A. Psychological disturbance is predicted to be positively correlated with perception of the therapist as similar to the mother. B. Psychological disturbance is predicted to be negatively correlated with perception of the therapist as similar to the father. C. Higher-functioning clients are predicted to view the therapist as more similar to the parent of the same sex as the therapist. 30 D. Lower-functioning clients are expected to perceive the therapist, regardless of gender, as more like the mother than the father. Level of personality functioning, or psychological disturbance, was measured by subjects' number of Rorschach criteria scores at initial testing. Subjects were then ranked on the number of criteria and grouped into higher—functioning (fewer criteria) and lower- functioning (greater number of criteria) subgroups. Hypotheses A and B were tested by using Pearson correlation coefficients to determine the relationship between number of criteria and distance between parents' and therapist's ICL scores. C and D were tested by employing the phi coefficient to measure the relationship between therapist gender and gender of the parent to whom the therapist is viewed as more similar. 4. In late therapy, the representation of the therapist will become significantly less similar than it had in mid-therapy to the parental representation it had been more similar to in mid-therapy. This increase in distance between therapist and parental representations was believed to demonstrate a resolution of some transferential issues and growing ability to view therapist, as well as others, in a more objective fashion. This hypothesis was tested by means of T-tests to determine whether a significant difference existed between the distance between the representation of the therapist and the mother at mid-therapy and at late therapy for subjects who perceived the therapist as similar to the mother in mid-therapy. Identical operations were performed for therapist and father representations in mid-therapy and late therapy 31 for subjects perceiving the therapist as like the father in mid- therapy. The following hypotheses regarding the outcome of therapy were also tested: 5. The total amount of movement in representations of self, therapist, mother, and father - that is, the sum of the deviations between ICL representations at early and mid-therapy and mid- and late therapy will be positively correlated with changes in personality functioning measured on the Rorschach from early to late therapy. The total amount of movement in each representation was measured by calculating the geometric distance between the representation at each testing and summing these distances. This score was then correlated using Pearson two-tailed correlations coefficients, with the following measures of personality functioning: The difference between hostility scores, anxiety scores, mean object representation scores, and criteria for personality functioning scores between Rorschachs administered early and late in therapy. 6. Significant differences on the Developmental Analysis of the Concept of the Object scales will be found on the overall global scale and each of the six subscales between initial and late Rorschach protocols. Paired T—tests were employed separately for the global and each of the subscales to test this hypothesis. 7. The number of representations at “moderate" intensity on the ICL in late therapy will be positively correlated with the Rorschach overall mean object relations score in late therapy. 32 This hypothesis was tested with the Pearson correlation coefficient. CHAPTERB Subjects Sixteen women receiving individual psychotherapy services at the Michigan State University Counseling Center during the 1984-85 academic year served as subjects for this study. Therapists were senior staff, interns, and practicum students at the Counseling Center to whom clients had been assigned according to standard center procedures. The length of therapy contact.was at least six months, but not more than one academic year. All female students receiving psychotherapy at the Olin branch of the Counseling Center, who were expected to remain in therapy for at least six months, and were working with the present therapist for the first time, were asked to participate. Twenty-four subjects initially agreed to participate. TWo subjects terminated therapy within the first three weeks, before any data was collected. Three subjects terminated therapy before the sixth session, so that only initial test data was available. Three more subjects terminated prior to the sixteenth session, so that only initial and mid-therapy data was available. Subjects and Therapists Subjects' ages ranged from twenty to thirty-four, with a mean age at initiation of psychotherapy of twenty—five. Fourteen had never 33 34 married, and two were divorced. Three subjects, including the two who had been divorced, had one or two children between the ages of two and ten. Six subjects were currently enrolled in master's or doctoral level graduate programs, and the other ten were undergraduates with sophomore, junior, and senior status. One subject had a history of substance abuse that included treatment at a residential facility. Seven subjects had received previous outpatient treatment of at least three months' duration. A number of subjects had past histories of significant separations, losses, and traumas. Family histories included two subjects whose fathers were survivors of concentration camps. One subject's parents had separated and later reconciled, but six subjects' parents had divorced, with five of the divorces taking place in the subjects' adolescences. One subject's father had permanently abandoned herself and her mother within a short time of her birth. Family psychiatric histories included two chronically mentally ill family members who had been hospitalized over extended periods of time. One subject's mother, and one's mother and sister had made suicidal gestures and attempts, one had herself made a suicide attempt, and one had an alcoholic parent. Past traumatic events for subjects included two incidences of physical abuse, two cases of sexual abuse by the father, two cases of childhood sexual abuse by non-family members, and one case of date rape in young adulthood. Presenting problems in fall 1984 nearly unanimously centered around general dissatisfaction with significant relationships, particularly'with parents and boyfriends. Most subjects complained of symptomatic depression and anxiety, variously described as 35 uncontrollable mood swings, ”being manic-depressive" by self- description rather than clinical diagnosis, and a number of different descriptions of a sense of dramatically increasing tension, stress, and hopelessness which had been unresponsive to the well—meaning effbrts at intervention made by family and friends. A few identified academic concerns as contributing to other problems, one had a psychologically related gastrointestinal illness, and one pinpointed the process of emotional separation from the family of origin as causing her distress. A total of ten therapists worked with the sixteen clients. Four therapists were female, and six were male, resulting in seven same-sex therapist-client dyads and nine opposite-sex therapist-client dyads. Three therapists (one female, two male) were in their first year of practicum, one male was in his second year of practicum, three female and two male therapists were Counseling Center interns, and one male therapist was a senior member of the Counseling Center staff. Premature Terminators Post-hoc analyses were carried out to determine whether any significant differences existed between those subjects who prematurely terminated therapy and those who did not. Prematurely terminating subjects' ages ranged from nineteen to twenty-five, with a mean age of 21.7. Five had never married, one was currently married, and none had children. All were undergraduates, although one had already completed a first bachelor's degree at a university in another country. One had a past history of psychiatric hospitalization for a previous suicide attempt, and three had histories of previous individual and family therapy outpatient treatment. Two had made past suicide attempts without subsequent hospitalization. Separations and losses for 36 premature terminators included one subject whose parents separated and reconciled, and one whose father died during her adolescence. Family psychiatric histories included one chronically alcoholic parent, one sibling whose family was reacting with turmoil to his homosexuality, and one father who, while married to the subject's mother, was flagrantly involved with tie subject's high school classmate. Presenting problems were identical to those of subjects who completed therapy, with the addition of one subject who reported binge eating. The subjects were seen by male therapists, and four saw female therapists. All of the therapists also had clients who completed this study. Two were in the first year of practicum, one was in the second year, and three were interns. Table 1 gives descriptive statistics for both premature terminators and completers on all initial testing variables as well as the magnitude of the difference between mean scores for the groups. No significant differences were noted between initial ICL scores. Premature terminators had significantly higher mean object relations scores, and significantly more Rorschach FC scores, than completers. There was also a trend toward terminators producing more reflection responses and a lower proportion of good quality pure form responses. Instruments The central data for this study consisted of variables drawn from Rorschach protocols administered according to the Exner (1974) system, the Interpersonal Checklist (LaForge & Suczek, 1955; Leary, 1956), and the Helping Relationship Questionnaire (Luborsky, 1984). The dynamic processes involved in tle production of each Rorschach response were elaborated by Rapaport, Gill & Schafer (1968) , Table 1 37 Thtests of Differences between PrematurelyaTerminatipg Clients and Clients Completipg Therapy on Initial Vafiables Clients Clients not completing completing therapy therapy N=16 Nel6 variable Mean SD Mean SD '3 Early therapy ICL: Self dominance 49.75 7.43 49.83 17.23 -.02 Self affiliation 51.81 6.96 53.50 14.35 -.38 Therapist dominance 61.38 4.91 62.50 8.02 -.40 Therapist affiliation 49.94 4.02 51.17 9.37 -.44 Mother dominance 57.38 6.62 63.00 12.46 -1.39 Mother affiliation 46.19 10.26 48.67 15.83 -.44 Father dominance 60.20 6.61 63.17 12.73 -.71 Father affiliate 37.33 7.30 40.00 9.76 -.69 Early therapy Rorschach: Rorschach criteria 7.31 2.44 6.67 1.9 .66 Mean object representation 9.24 1.59 11.45 2.87 -2.32* Hostility content 3.13 2.53 5.33 3.45 -1.66 Anxiety content 4.75 5.69 3.33 2.50 .81 R 24.81 10.20 26.67 8.21 -.44 Location: W’ 11.69 7.24 15.83 7.19 -1.20 D 9.31 6.97 7.17 4.40 .49 Dd 3.56 3.48 3.67 2.16 -.08 S 2.31 2.21 4.17 2.93 .20 Determinants: FM 3.06 2.91 2.83 2.04 .21 m 1.06 1.29 2.00 1.55 -1.44 M 5.44 3.48 5.67 1.63 -.21 FC 2.56 1.71 4.50 1.38 -2.74** CF + C 2.12 2.03 2.67 2.94 -.49 FC' + C'F + C' .75 1.00 1.33 1.51 -1.06 FT + TF + F 1.19 1.80 2.17 2.64 -1.00 FV + VF +‘V .31 .48 .50 1.23 -.53 FY + YF + Y 3.31 3.42 3.00 3.69 .19 Fr + rF .31 .87 1.33 1.51 -2.00* (2) 7.25 3.17 6.50 1.05 .83 FD 1.19 1.42 .50 .55 1.63 F 8.50 4.34 6.67 3.62 1.00 (table continues) 38 Clients Clients not completing completing therapy therapy N=16 N:16 variable Mean SD Mean SD .E Percentages & ratios: E 9.44 5.55 11.00 3.08 -.83 es 9.75 6.02 11.33 5.50 -.59 D score -1.81 1.94 -1.83 .41 -.04 F+% .60 .20 .46 .12 2.00* X+% .64 .13 .55 .09 1.74 X-% .05 .05 .04 .04 .59 3r + (2)/ R .34 .13 .38 .69 -.92 Lambda .64 .52 .39 .19 .26 S-constellation 3.34 1.93 3.50 1.38 -.17 Afr .65 .41 .52 .16 1.11 * p < .10. ** E < .05. 39 we see three prominent phases in the process of the coming about of a Rorschach response: in the first phase, the salient perceptual features of the blot initiate the associative process: in the second, this process pushes beyond these partial perceptual impressions and effects a more or less intensive organization and elaboration of the inkblot; in the third, the perceptual potentialities and limitations of the inkblot act as a regulating reality for the associative process itself. Clearly, then, it would not be correct to reason that the Rorschach response is to be considered mainly either a perceptual product or one of free association. Either view“would fail to reflect the cogwheeling of the progress of perceptual organization with the associative process. (p. 276) The ICL, as summarized by Keisler (1983), is an interpersonal measurement instrument. The subject is asked to indicate on,a written form which, of a broad array of 128 attributes, are considered true for the person being rated. A formula is then applied to the attributions considered descriptive of the ratee, culminating in a single score being assigned to the ratee which describes their position in "a two- dimensional Euclidian space reflecting the joint action of two basic interpersonal dimensions or motivations, almost universally designated Control and Affiliation, These dimensions define, respectively, the vertical and horizontal axes of the circle” (Kiesler, 1983, p. 186). The final score, therefore, is a single numerical value which is the intersection of standard scores on the two axis dimensions, reflecting the rater's perception of the ratee. The degree of 'intensity' (moderate versus extreme) is determined by the distance of this intersection from the midpoint of the circumplex. Ratings falling within one standard deviation (mean=50, SD=10) are considered moderate, while ratings toward the periphery of the circumplex in any direction are considered extreme, implying rigidity of interpersonal behavior. 40 The Helping Relationship Questionnaire is a short, self-report measure designed by Luborsky (1984) to access the client's view of his or her experience in psychotl'erapy. This questionnaire consists of 11 statements answered on a Likert-style scale from ore (strongly disagree) to six (strongly agree) describing the client's views of how much change has occurred in tlerapy and the relationship with the therapist, and includes two open-ended questions about areas in which the client believes he or she has improved and not improved. Luborsky has used this questionnaire as a part of his larger investigation into both the degree to which supportive-expressive psychotherapy was performed by the psychotherapist and to measure the patient's experience of the helping alliance. All of these scales were presented in his 1984 book, which was subtitled, A manual for supportive- expressive treatment. Luborsky used the earliest forms of the manual to train his psychotherapy supervisees in the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Pennsylvania. Procedures Therapists were informed of the study and their participation was requested at initial staff and practicum meetings in September. During the first or second psychotlerapy session with clients meeting the inclusion criteria, the therapist read a short description of the study from the Therapist Information Form and asked the client if she would be interested in participating. Clients expressing interest were given the Initial Contact Sheet to complete. The therapist then returned this form to my mailbox. I called the client within two days and made an initial appointment for testing. Testing during the early phase of therapy 41 took place as soon after the first therapy session as possible, but was always completed prior to the fifth therapy session. I administered the majority of the early, mid- and late therapy testing. To minimize potential bias, an advanced graduate student, experienced in the Rorschach, administered early and late therapy tests to my own clients. Another advanced graduate student carried out three early therapy testing sessions. At the first testing, the study was described to the subject, stressing that there would be three test sessions and that participation had no bearing whatsoever on receiving counseling services. The subject first signed the consent form, gave some basic demographic infonmation, and was administered the Rorschach. When the Rorschach had been completed, the subject was presented with the ICL and offered the choice of completing it at once or completing it at home and returning it in its self-addressed, stamped envelope. The subject was instructed to read the list of statements, marking those which she believed were true for her and leaving blank those which were not true. After answering the statements for her perception of herself, she was instructed to read through the list again, answering them for her therapist, and to follow identical procedures for her mother and for her father. Participating therapists were periodically asked to supply the actual dates of therapy sessions that had taken place with research subjects. Mid-therapy testing was scheduled to take place between the tenth and thirteenth session. The subject was called after the ninth session, and a second appointment was made. During this session, the 42 ICL‘was administered in the same fashion as it has been given in the previous session. The final test administration was scheduled to take place as close as possible to the final therapy session, and in each case occurred ‘within the last four sessions of psychotherapy. Subjects were called and a final appointment.was made. The Rorschach was once again administered, with the added instruction that the subject should report what she saw, regardless of whether it.was the same or different than what was seen at initial testing. The IC1.was administered next, in standard fashion, followed by the Helping Relationship Questionnaire. Scoring_Procedures Rorschach. Each Rorschach protocol was scored for structural variables according to Exner (1974). Eighteen of these variables and ratios and two scores added later by Exner (Exner, 1986) were used to construct a global measure of personality functioning. Using Exner's 1986 data drawn from 600 nonpatient adults (pp. 257-258) scores on 15 of these variables falling beyond one standard deviation from the mean were determined. The five ratios that were included were based on Exner's (1986) hypotheses regarding the meaning of directionality on these ratios in terms of adaptive ego functioning. Table 2 gives the variables and ranges of scores used. Using these guidelines, the number of criteria - the number of variables falling outside nonpatient normative performance - was determined. A high number of criteria scores indicated greater deviation from nonpatient norms, while a low number of criteria scores indicated increased similarity to the nonpatient population. Subjects were ranked according to this scale. Using early therapy criteria 43 Table 2 Rorschach Scores Used as Criteria for Level of Personality Functionipg 1. M : Sum C 5_2.00 2. chcrwc 3. D score # 0 4. M S_FM + m 5. M— > 0 6. FC' + C'F + C' > 2 7. FT + TF + T > 2 8. FY + YF + Y > 2 9. FV + VF +‘V > 2 10. FD > 2 11. P < 5 or > 8 12. Lambda < .31 or > .87 13. X+% < 71 or > 89 14. X-% > 11 15. F+% < 59 or > 93 16. Zd >.: 3.5 17. Afr < .47 or > .85 18. Egocentricity index < .28 or > .50 19. S-constellation Z 8 20. S > 2 44 scores, the eight subjects receiving the lowest scores on this scale (low deviates from normative performance) were classified as higher- functioning or having less impairment of ego functioning. The eight receiving the highest scores (high deviation from normative performance = greater impairment of personality functioning) were classified as lower functioning. An advanced graduate student was trained to score each protocol for quality of object representation according to the Developmental Analysis of the Concept of the Object on the Rorschach scale (Blatt, ‘ \ Brennais, Schimek & Glick, 1975). This cognitive-structural scoring system is based on a theoretical integration of developmental theories of Heinz werner and Jean Piaget with psychoanalytic ego psychology (Blatt & Lerner, 1983). The scale is composed of six major subscales on which Rorschach responses with human content are scored along a developmental continuum. Subcategories in each subscale are weighted according to amount of development indicated. For each protocol, the total number of responses within each subcategory were tallied, the weighted sum of the subcategories was computed for each subscale, yielding a developmental index score for each subscale, and subscale developmental index scores were summed and divided by the total number of scorable responses, producing an overall mean object relations score. The same rater also scored each protocol for anxious and hostile content on the Elizur (1949; Aronow & Reznikoff, 1976) scales. These scales weight anxious and hostile content according to the intensity of affect expressed whether by behavior attributed to figures in the percept, symbolism in the percept, or the way in which the percept is 45 described. The weighted sum of scores for each protocol was then computed. [199. Subjects completed ICL ratings of four individuals - self, therapist, mother, and father - a total of three times. Two subjects said they had had so little contact with their biological father that it.was impossible to describe him, and instead completed ratings for their stepfathers, the men they considered to have been the father figure in their lives. One subject had never had any contact with either her biological father or any other male in a similar role, so she was instructed to disregard father ratings. Following standard procedures (Leary, 1956) the number of statements scored as "true” for the ratee were added separately for the 16 subscales, and subscale scores were combined in standard formulas which yielded raw scores on dominance and affiliation axes. The raw score for each axis was then compared to a table of standard scores. Once standard scores were obtained, the intersection of the two scores on the axes was plotted on a diagnostic grid or circumplex composed of eight discrete diagnostic octants. By noting which octant a representation lay within, and the distance of that representation from the midpoint, a diagnostic code was assigned, as well as whether that representation was ”moderate" or "extreme.” Measurement of the total amount of activity in representations of self and others was done by applying the formula dx + dy , where dx is the vertical discrepancy between representations and dy the horizontal. The formula, which measures the geometric distance between pairs of ratings, was applied to the distance between early and mid- therapy representations and mid- and late therapy representations, and 46 the sum of the two distances was obtained separately for each representation. CHAPTER 4 RESUDTS Main Hypotheses ayppthesis l. Representations of the self, the therapist, the mother, and the father will change significantly from early to mid- therapy and mid— to late therapy. Significant differences were found between early and mid-therapy for the amount of affiliation in the self representation, the intensity of the dominance in the self representation, and the amount of dominance in the maternal representation. The self representation became significantly less affiliative again from mid- to late therapy. The intensity of self-dominance increased significantly in mid-therapy, while the amount of dominance attributed to the mother decreased sharply in mid-therapy. Table 3 presents the mean representation scores at each stage of therapy. Hypothesis 2. In mid-therapy the representation of the therapist will become significantly more similar to the representation of one parent than it had been in early therapy. The deviation between parental and therapist representations failed to decrease significantly between early and mid-therapy either for the nine subjects perceiving the therapist as more similar to 47 48 .Ho. wrm.uo pancamacmam mmcaumu mmmumoulcfle can Manse consume monoHoMMflp u m .mo. wlm.uo untowMflcmHm mmCHumu mmmuoca coca can ammumcunofle consume oocouomwwc u N .mo. Iv. m an unmoflwcmam mmfiumu Eggnog can 3.8m smegma cocouowmac u H genumnuauua mN.mH mm.mH oo.mH vv.m Hm.h vm.h vv.¢ oo.m mo.m vv.v oo.m mo.m umvflmcmucH oocmcaeoo mH.NH mm.HH mm.NH vv.o ma.h mm.h mm.HH mm.¢ mm.HH Hm.o mwo.m mm.v uhnflmcmucH no.hm mm.wm mm.hm mo.Hm Hm.m¢ ma.o¢ Hm.m¢ oo.mv vm.mv Nam.om Nmm.mv Hm.Hm COHumfiHHmm< hm.mm ma.mm oN.oo mH.Nm Hmm.om mm.hm mm.Ho mm.am mm.Ho Hm.om Hm.om mh.mv OUCMCHEOQ 8.3 be; s28 83 92 s28 .83 2: 38d 83 e2 bums End 850: umflmmumfi 2mm mmmmum acumen. 33 can .083: .bumm 5 Moscow com Josue: .umflmmuorfi. dam now mouoom coflumucomoumom .HUH coo: m manna 49 mother or the six subjects perceiving the therapist as more similar to the father. Hypothesis 2 was not supported. Hypothesis 3. In mid-tterapy, A. Psychological disturbance will be positively correlated with perception of the therapist as similar to the mother. B. Psychological disturbance will be negatively correlated with perception of the therapist as similar to tte father. C. Higher-functiom’ng clients will view tte tterapist as more similar to the parent of the same sex as tte therapist. D. lower-functioning clients will see tte tterapist as more like the father than the mother, regardless of therapist gender. Adaptive functioning was strongly correlated with perceiving both mother and father as similar to the therapist. The correlation between number of criteria for psychopathology and motter-therapist deviation was .64 (p = .004) . Fetter-therapist deviation and Rorschach criteria were correlated at a level of .52 (p = .02). 2 x 2 tables for C and D yielded significant results, with phi coefficient values of .55 (df = 6) and .26 (df = 7) both significant at p_<_ .01. Hypothesis 4. In late therapy, the representation of the therapist will become significantly more deviant than it had been in mid-therapy to tte parental representation it had been more similar to in mid-therapy. Therapist and mother representations were more distant, but not to a significant extent, at late therapy for subjects seeing the therapist as more similar to mother than fatter in mid-therapy. A trend was found for the distance between fatter and therapist representations to 50 increase sizably (pug .10) from mid- to late therapy for subjects perceiving the therapist as more similar to father than to mother. Hypothesis 5. The total amount of activity or movement in representations of self, therapist, mother, and father will be positively correlated with the difference in pre- and post-therapy hostility content, anxiety content, mean overall object representation, and level of functioning criterion Rorschach scores. Table 4 gives the mean Rorschach for these variables at both stages of therapy, and Table 5 presents the correlations obtained between these Rorschach variables and the ICL activity variables. A significant relationship was noted between the total amount of activity in ICL representations and changes in Rorschach content. These content scores are actually difference scores, the difference between hostility and anxiety upon entering and upon exiting psychotherapy. The amount of anxiety and hostility content actually increased rather than decreased throughout the course of therapy. No significant correlations were obtained between the total amount of movement and the pre-post differences in mean object representation or number of criteria scores. Hypothesis 6. There will be a significant difference between the overall mean object relations scores and each of the six subscale scores on the Developmental Analysis of the Concept of the Object scale between pre- and post-therapy Rorschachs. Table 6 present means, standard deviations, correlations, and E values for the subscales and the overall scale. Tl 51 Table 4 Mean Early and Late-therapy Rorschach Hostility, Anxiety, Object Representation, and Criteria Scores Early Late Hostility 3.12 4.00 Anxiety 4.75 7.06 Object Representation 9.24 9.32 Criteria 7.31 7.37 Table 5 Pearson Correlations between Total Amount of Change in ICL Representations of Self, TherapISt, Mother, and Father and Total Change from Early to Late Rorschach Hostility, Anxiety, Mean Object Representation and Criteria Scores Rorschach Score ICL Object Representation Hostility Anxiety Representation Criteria Self .15 .43* .25 .11 Therapist .52* .47* .21 .12 Mother .53* .45* .18 .19 Father .48* .14 -.20 -.07 * = p_< .05. 52 Table 6 T-tests Comparing Rorschach Object Representation Scores in Early and in Late Therapy Early to Late Developmental Early Late Mean t Index Mean SD Mean SD Difference value Differentiation 23.19 10.82 24.31 13.22 1.13 -.51 Articulation 17.71 10.47 18.38 9.89 .63 -.21 Motivation Object-action Integration 9.38 6.51 9.81 7.95 .44 -.21 Nature of Action 10.25 6.18 10.31 5.93 .06 -.04 content of Interaction 6.56 4.08 6.44 3.86 .13 .11 Overall Mean 53 No significant differences between the overall score or subscale scores were obtained. The majority of E values did, however, fall in the predicted direction. For all but one subscale, small increases in capacity for object relations were found in late therapy. Although most differences between scores are so small as to prohibit assuming clinical meaning, the fact that all but one lie in the expected direction argues against random effects and offers some‘weak support for the validity of this scale and for the occurrence of some slight increase in capacity for object relations with therapy. Hypothesis 7. The number of representations at moderate intensity in late therapy will be positively correlated with the late therapy Rorschach object relations score. The correlation between these variables was .24 (p.= .19) and thus this hypothesis was not confirmed. Rorschach Table 7 gives the correlation coefficients between first and second administrations for each Rorschach variable. Testrretest correlations were high for most variables, suggesting that perception and response tendencies indicated on these protocols reflect characteristic modes of functioning for these subjects. compared to Exner's 1986 onedyear and three-year test-retest correlations for nonpatient adults, however, the coefficients obtained in this study are strikingly low. Exner established the range of .80 and above as determining internal consistency, and his nonpatient samples produced coefficients ranging from .70 to .91 for the majority of variables. His 1978 study of 30 long-term psychotherapy cases retested after approximately 180 days yielded coefficients comparable to those in this 54 Table 7 Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Rorschach variables between First and Second Administrations Present Exner Study (1978) N216 Ne30 Variable Description E p _r; R NUmber of responses .83 .001 .64 W Number of whole responses .76 .001 D Number of usual detail responses .77 .001 Dd Number of unusual detail responses .46 .04 S Number of white space responses .48 .03 M Number of human movement responses .58 .009 .68 PM Number of animal movement responses .26 .17 .49 m Number of inanimate movement responses .51 .02 FC Number of form dominant responses .72 .001 .39 CF + C Number of color dominant responses .27 .16 .47 FC' + C'F + C' Number of achromatic color responses .06 .41 FT + TF + T Number of texture responses .80 .001 FY + YF + Y Number of diffuse shading responses .73 .001 FV +‘VF + v NUmber of vista responses .56 .012 Fr + rF NUmber of reflection responses .67 .002 FD Number of form-based dimensional responses .36 .08 F NUmber of pure form responses .67 .002 .51 Percentages and ratios: es EXperienced stimulation (PM + m + T + C' +‘V + Y) .62 .006 .61 D score Stress tolerance index (Scaled EA-es difference score) .34 .10 3r + (2) / r Egocentricity index .72 .001 .73 Lambda Number of pure form/number of non-pure form responses .54 .015 .48 F+% Good quality pure form .25 .19 X+% Extended good form .45 .04 .81 Xe% Poor form .34 .10 S-constellation .34 .10 EA Experience actual (M + weighted sum C) .78 .001 .70 Afr Affective ratio .73 .001 .73 55 study. The similarity between this group and Exner's treatment group, and tte difference between these groups and Ebmer's nonpatient sample seem to demonstrate tte impact of six months of psychotherapy on personality variables that would have most likely remained unchanged without psychotherapy. The implication for this study is that psychotherapy may be shown to have had a significant influence on aspects of personality functioning, and that tte sample in this study appears to be comparable with other samples. Another comparison between normative groups and the sample in this study is shown by Table 8, which presents descriptive statistics on forty Rorschach variables and ratios for initial and second administrations for this study and for Emer's 1986 data on nonpatient adults and outpatient adults diagnosed as character problems. Initial Rorschach means for subjects in this study seem to fall somewhere between those of the two Exner groups, confirming that tte level of psychotherapy for this group seems to be well within tte neurotic range, neither quite as well cognitively and affectively organized as the nonpatients, nor as disorganized as the character problems. Helping Relationship Questionnaire Table 9 shows the descriptive statistics for this instrument. The majority of subjects agreed to each of tte items measuring such qualitatively different aspects of general improvetent as feeling helped, obtaining self-understanding, working jointly with tte therapist, and increased autonomy. All clients indicated they believed they had improved, with tte modal response being ”much improved.” A post-hoc correlation between subjects' estimates of improvement and level of personality functioning was performed. No relationship was 56 HamscHucoo «Hams. m~ o oH AH.m ~m.a mH H a s~.m AH.o mH o m vH.m oo.o «H o mH en.o om.m a s o c HH.H ac.o a o H so.H mH.H o o o mm.H so.H c o o ~4.H mH.H on e o o vm.c Hv.o e o o me.o NH.o m o c a~.H ms.o n o o as. Hm.o mu+uu H~ o o mv.~ mm.o AH H a mm.~ ve.m AH n s ss.n «a.» HH n m RH.m m~.s .m. o c N HH.H nm.~ OH O n vo.~ oH.e HH H m oa.~ om.m cH n m RH.n ve.m : a c o HH.H nH.H oH o o oo.H om.o HH H H v~.m ms.n SH a o ~v.n Hn.m ».m>.sm n a o am.c m~.o m o o mm.o we.o H o o cm.o mH.o H c o o..o Hn.o >.m>.>u m c o so.o vv.o m o H oo.o oH.H MH c c ce.n mH.~ m o c om.H aH.~ a.se.ea m o a HH.H os.c cH o H c~.H Hm.H m c o am.H mo.H m o o oo.H ms.o .o+m.o..mo n o o eo.~ me.H A o n vo.H oH.~ s c H Hs.H em.H o o H nc.~ ~H.~ o+mo m a o mn.H sm.o a c m oc.~ sm.~ o o ,N Hm.~ om.~ o o m HA.H em.~ on m o o HH.H no.H a o H eo.H m~.H m o o «m.H oo.H v o o o~.H mo.H a A o H ne.H u~.~ a H . Hm.H Hm.n «H H H HH.H 4m.n HH o H Ho.“ oo.m 2m mvgwug s o H oo.H Hm.H a o H mo.H 4m.H s o o m¢.~ oo.m a o o H~.~ Hm.~ m a c o sm.~ mm.~ vH o o es.~ ms.H mH o o Hm.e Ha.4 «H o o o¢.n om.n no H~ a v Hm.v No.» v~ m 4H 45.. mm.~H mm o cc.n an.» ms.a H~ a m sm.o Hm.m o mm H o ea.m mm.» o~ ~ a Go.~ cm.o RN m oo.m mm.m mo.oH mm m nH ¢~.A ae.HH : coHuaooH on oH AH Hv.m mm.sH an oH nw vm.m sm.- so a oo.HH HH.HH Hm.m~ av HH «H o~.oH Hm.v~ a muamHum> xa: :Hz moo: am can: xa: :Hz use: on cam: xa: cH: moo: am can: xm: 2H: one: am can: oowuz mfiwmnoum Hupuoumgu acmHHaduso oowmz muasc‘ ucmwumacoz Homon. umcxw cofiumuumMCHect Honda coHumHuchHEo< HuHuHcH eHuz spasm Hammond weBchS 209.089. no.“ mega—coupon can 8H3: 4moHumHH3w gHUAmHBB u wanna 57' m.mm HHH N.mH oHH m.NH N m.NH N N .v. 2 0H8 m.Hv 8 HH.H o c o n6 H m.H A 3953 m.mN HmH HH.N NH :6... a cém a mm. v H: m.mN Hm n6 mm a v m.nv N l on. v :22 + an HH.N v o.H HH c o c o o A 3 8333330 m HH.N. on n.n ON n6 H n6 H H. A a I x o.m¢ co H.oN NH .73 a o.nN HH ON. v o + m mév mm o.oH ow oéo HH c.mN NH cN. v a + x o.vm no c.HN oNH can a 99 N o m 0.80m o m.mH an oén wHN m6 H n6 H 339.3388. N A 8.959 o.om NHH o.nN 9H m.Nn o 9.2 N 3:335: HH.H: 3 TH”; mJN Ho H6. HvN m6... a o.om a HgHuuvsoHucH. N A 9591. o aucusvoum a aucmswopm a accusoohm a mucuswowm o o v SH 84 m o c HH.N No.N a N N vH.H om.n N o m 34 qmé cooum NHH HN.o mm... 36 Hm.o NH 36 on... «H... 36 m H H mm. SH 34 NN... Nw.o Hvé was but H... o oo.H nv.H Hm.H mN.N 36 No... cN.o $5 oN.H o o mmé omé HH.N o o 36 $5 mos—.3 mm... o and 36 Ne... 36 no... Nmé HH.H. on... Hmé 86 8.0 86 :6 and 35 Nv.o 35 end H.223.” mad 0 o mod mH.o 96 o 36 mo... ooé 26 o 0 mod 36 36 o c 3... 8.0 aux oo.H on.c mN.° nH.o ON.c oo.H ov.c wa.o mo.o om.c H¢.o mm.o mm.o HH.o Nm.o mm.c om.o vo.o nH.o vo.o o+x oo.H c o.H oHé HN.c oo.H c 8H :6 2.6 8H 35 cm... 26 $6 c.H NH.o $6 35 86 f... N 7. o 8H 8.9: H n: o 3H 36 n m: c 3H NHH: v m: H: 3H 8.? 988 o 3 o v 3:.” $6 Hm m a $4 HN.m em N N :6 8.3 HN H 0 Ned mN.m mo m.vH o N SN :4 N m.H a me «N... NN H N 2...... 36 RN m.N om.m mmfi 31m 5 moHumm H «mandamOHQm 3.5.88. 8:8: xifizooflomcmmz 33:89. 858: 35:30: 8 :8: oomlz mEanoum “Song £83.83 oouuz munzut ucwwumaccz .wmmH. hmcxm EHuahchHefi H2: Bumbag HaHuHcH 2.: exam anemone 58 Table 9 Descriptive Statistics for the Helping Relationship Questionnaire l = strongly disagree 2 = disagree 3 = mildly disagree 4 = mildly agree 5 = agree 6 = strongly agree Item Mean Mode SD Range 1. I believe that my therapist is helping me. 5.19 5 0.75 4-6 2. I believe that the treatment is helping me. 5.19 5 0.75 4-6 3. I have obtained some new understanding. 5.50 6 0.63 4-6 4. I have been feeling better recently. 4.44 4 1.09 2-6 5. I can already see that I will eventually work out the problems I came to treatment for. 4.75 4 1.34 1-6 6. I feel I can depend upon the therapist. 4.75 4 1.29 3-6 7. I feel the therapist understands me. 4.88 5 0.89 3-6 8. I feel the therapist wants me to achieve my goals. 5.63 6 0.62 4-6 9. I feel I am working together with the therapist in a joint effort. 5.06 6 0.85 4-6 10. I believe we have similar ideas about the nature of of my problems. 4.88 5 0.62 4-6 11. I feel now that I can understand myself and deal with myself on my own. 4.19 4 1.33 2-6 Total of items 1 - 11 54.44 59 7.16 40-65 Self-estimate of improvement (l=not at all, 2=slightly, 3=moderately, 4=much, 5=very much) 3.56 4 1.03 2-5 59 found between the total HRQ score (total of items 1 - 11) and number of Rorschach criteria on either initial or second administrations (£_= o and .25 respectively), indicating that subjects' level of pathology was unrelated to their subjective experience of improvement. CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION The seven hypotheses address two general issues: (1) whether there are distinct phases in therapy and the therapeutic relationship in which self and others are experienced in subjectively different ways, with particular attention to correlates of the quality of the transference around the time it is hypothesized to be most intense, and (2) how a number of personality features change across the course of therapy. General Issues An immediate issue regarding the cumulative findings from this study is that the low number of subjects prevents any definite conclusions from being drawn. The low'N contributed to the difficulty of attaining statistical significance. It also decreased the likelihood that the findings of this study are true for the population, increasing the likelihood that chance factors operated to confirm or disprove hypotheses. ‘While the high possibility of random effects must be taken seriously while evaluating these results, other factors also merit consideration. Buckley, Conte, Plutchik, Wild and Karasu (1984) addressed similar methodology issues and claimed that in exploratory research, such as their study of therapy outcome and the present study, Type I errors were more acceptable than Type II errors. 60 61 Auerbach mingled attitudes of scientific skepticism and pragmatism in his 1983 paper on assessment of therapy outcome. He took the position that, Outcome should be viewed like the concept of a true score in classical test theory; it is known only to God. Our measurements of outcome are approximations, and because of the nature of personality and of psychotherapy, they are a good deal less precise than tte determination of IQ...We must do that.which is feasible and seems reasonable while remembering that the appearance of reasonableness may be deceptive. (p. 538) Auerbach articulated two primary attitudes toward psychotherapy outcome research: 'lifelike complexity' and 'reliability,' each of which had specific advantages and disadvantages. 'Lifelike complexity' was described as more ambitious, usually involving explanation as well as description, but also being significantly more difficult. On that continuum, this study clearly falls on the side of complexity. It was intended to offer in rich clinical description.what it lacked in quantitative rigor and validity. Length of Treatment. One strength of this study is its duration of six to ten months of therapy. Buckley et al. (1984) pointed out that the vast majority of psychotherapy studies have involved periods of treatment of less than two months. The two most highly regarded and oft-cited outcome studies, the Psychotherapy Project of the Menninger Foundation and the Penn Psychotherapy Project, studied much longer courses of therapy, from 44 to 800 hours of treatment. Between the two extremes, little literature exists on relatively brief, but not explicitly short-term, dynamic psychotherapy. Shapiro and Shapiro's 1982 meta-analysis of therapy outcome studies found relatively few studies using dynamic and humanistic methods of treatment. Even.when all studies from these perspectives 62 were combined - a conceptually'weak assumption - the majority of such studies used group rather than individual treatment, therapy lasted a mean of eleven hours, was carried out by therapists with a mean 3.7 years of experience, and only one-third of the data was collected in clinical settings. Subject Characteristics. Demographic and other attributes of this sample strengthen the relevance of this study to general clinical situations. The mean age of subjects, the high proportion of graduate students, the inclusion of subjects with children, and the severity of some subjects' psychiatric histories argue that this sample bore more resemblance to a general outpatient population than to that of a nonpatient university undergraduate population. Inspection of descriptive statistics for Rorschach variables for this group compared to Exner's nonpatient and outpatient samples further supported this conclusion. Buckley et a1. (1984) found similar outpatient-nonpatient. comparisons in their medical student population. Their therapy patients also had measurably greater psychopathology than their nonpatients, leading the authors to conclude that in this high- functioning clinical sample the concerns leading subjects to seek treatment were the result of psychopathology which would have been manifested, and led them to seek treatment, in any setting. This has relevance for this sample as well. Although they were necessarily functioning well enough to remain in college, the kinds and severity of presenting problems and history suggests that these were mild to moderate neurotic and character problems which would have led them to 63 seek outpatient treatment whether they were enrolled in school or currently employed. Analyses of several hypotheses compared higher~ and lower- functioning subgroups. Although the total sample divided neatly into two groups of eight, differences in severity between the subgroups may have been slight. One reason for this was subject selection. Clinical needs took clear precedence over research, and in all cases in which recruitment.was feared to interfere with the client's ability to feel safe in therapy, with development of the therapeutic relationship, or where the client appeared too agitated, no recruitment todk place. This may well have eliminated many clients whose more severe psychopathology caused them to present to the counseling Center in crisis, and whose diminished trust and investment in others may have led them to refuse to participate even if approached. The high percentage of graduate students indicates an opposite effect. These tended to be women who had personal experience‘with research, had themselves undergone the tedious process of gathering data, several of whom verbalized attitudes of pleasure in participating in the academic community through such activities. Two additional factors might account for some of the negative findings in this study. The first factor is the therapists' level of experience. With.the exception of one experienced faculty member, the remaining therapists had no more than three years of prior experience practicing psychotherapy. A substantial number of the cases studied was the therapists' first cases of their careers. As stated above, this level of training is commonly found in psychotherapy process and —""1-1l 64 outcome studies, but suggests that tte impact of tte psychotterapeutic process might well have been stronger‘with more seasoned clinicians. The second factor is the adequacy of the measuring instruments. The ICL and the Rorschach are standard instruments (particularly the Rorschach), but are not often applied in exactly these ways. conceptually, both measures seemed sufficient to measure the phenomena in question. As they have not been previously applied in just this way, however, it is entirely possible they simply failed to indicate changes that did, in fact, occur. One of the post-hoc analyses concerned differences between subjects who completed the course of psychotherapy, and those who terminated prematurely. There were no differences between these groups on their representations of themselves, their therapists, their fathers, or their mothers, although there was a nonsignificant tendency for terminators to perceive both mother and father as more dominant and more affiliative than completers. Significant differences appeared in Rorschach variables between the two groups. Terminators had higher mean object relations scores, a greater number of FC responses, a higher number of reflection responses, and a lower percentage of good quality pure form responses. EXner (1986) considered reflections to be indicative of a degree of egocentricity or self-centeredness, and the mean for terminators was significantly above that of Exner's nonpatients. Terminators seemed to have a higher general level of functioning in capacity to relate to others and to manage affect, but to feel acutely uncomfortable, self-conscious, and perhaps withdrawn from forming affective connections with others, possibly including the therapist. Their higher level of general functioning may have led them 65 to feel less pressure to make the necessary commitment to continue in psychotherapy, and their possible tendency to bend reality may have aided them in distorting either their view of what occurred in therapy, the realities of their current situation, or both. The terminators' relative youth in comparison to completers may also have led them to overestimate the situational nature of their difficulties, and to underestimate the contribution of internal conflicts in bringing about current distress. The Relationship Between Change in Representations and Change in Rorschach Content Hypothesis 5 stated that the total amount of change (activity or movement) in each representation would be positively correlated with indicators of personality functioning. The underlying rationale for this hypothesis was that at the conclusion of successful psychodynamic psychotherapy the client would experience lessened symptomatology, as measured by Rorschach content scores, increased capacity for object relatedness, and more adaptive use of inner resources, as measured by Rorschach criteria scores. These changes in functioning were believed to be directly tied to the changes in internal representations of self and others which were expected to occur in therapy through the corrective emotional experience with the therapist, interpretation and exploration of the transference itself, uncovering of earlier memories, particularly those involving early interactions*with the parents, and exploration of current interpersonal relationships. The results of the correlations between ttese scores were mixed. Significant results were obtained for hostility and anxiety content, while the other two scores were unrelated to ICL activity. Self-report [1.1.1.7. - 66 and projective measures have often failed to be significantly related even when they have been believed to be conceptually similar. A gereral 1y accepted explanation has been tte proposition that ttese tap different levels of depth in tte personality, whetter tte levels are called unconscious and preconscious, or Ieary's (1956) Levels 11 (private self-description) and III (fantasy and underlying character structure). As content scores changed significantly from early to late therapy, while adaptive functioning (as measured by Rorschach criteria scores) did not, this suggests that tte content scores may measure an aspect of personality less fundamental than underlying personality structure itself, but deeper than conscious knowledge of self and others. It also suggests that symptomatic relief may substantially precede structural personality change in psychotterapy. Hostility, Anxiety, and Level of Functionirg The relationship between hostility and anxiety change highlights the relationship between level of functioning, anxiety, and hostility. Post-hoc analyses found a very near significant (p = .059, .055) negative correlation between initial level of functioning and initial and final levels of hostility, and a significant (p = .04) positive correlation between initial level of functioning and late therapy anxiety. Better-functioning subjects had higher levels of hostility at tte beginning and end of therapy. It was otherwise lower-functioning clients whose hostility was expressed in more disguised forms, possibly resulting in lower hostility scores. Early and late therapy hostility and anxiety were negatively correlated (_r; = -.16, -.l9) but not to a significant extent. While tte correlation between initial anxiety and level of functioning was very slight (_r_ = .08, p = .39), subjects who 67 were better-functioning at the onset of therapy had significantly lower levels of anxiety at the end of therapy than lower-functioning subjects. Rabinowitz (1975) proposed that hostility ratings were primarily a measure of the individual's awareness of his or her inner self and hailingness to accept hostile feelings and attitudes. He found that the ability to fantasize was a major source of variation among several hostility measures. A growing awareness or tolerance of one's own hostility would explain the increase in hostility scores observed in this study, but a post-hoe analysis failed to produce significant correlations between initial hostility, late therapy hostility, the difference between these scores, and the Rorschach M response, long considered an index of capacity for fantasy and inner life. Change in the Capacitygfor Object Relations The increase in differentiation at the end of therapy’was so small as to be statistically insignificant. The most likely explanation for the small size of this increase is the form of therapy received by the subject. Previous reports of this scale in the literature have cited changes in differentiation following intensive, long-term psychoanalytic psychotherapy. The modal form of therapy practiced at the Counseling Center is a shorter-term, psychodynamic psychotherapy typically involving one weekly session. The differences in length, intensity, and goals of treatment probably accounted for the more modest improvement attained in this study. The results of Hypotheses 5 and 6 together strongly suggest that in the six to ten months of psychodynamic psychotherapy, changes in the handling and expression of unpleasant affects took place, representing the uncovering and more "31 fans. n‘n ‘ 68 adaptive handling of previously repressed material, but only minimal change in basic character and personality structure took place. Quality of the Transference This study was designed to measure perceptions of self and others at three phases which were believed to reflect discrete stages in the therapeutic relationship. The early or initial phase of therapy was believed to be one in which the client's original, distorted perceptions of self and others were as yet unchanged, and the perception of the therapist was relatively neutral. By the midpoint of therapy it was expected that the process of gradual exploration would have brought many of these distorted perceptions into greater awareness, and the cumulative effect of the experience with the therapist would have resulted in a transference reaction with predominantly paternal or maternal features. At the end of therapy, with the termination process encouraging resolution of conflicts and disengagement from the therapist, it.was predicted that the perception of the therapist would be more neutral and less transferentially colored than in mid-therapy. Because previous studies had indicated that the developmental phase at which the analysis was being conducted influenced the maternal or paternal quality of the transference, differences between clients were expected in accord with level of psychosocial development and probable psychosexual fixation. A strong correlation was found between therapist-mother and therapist-father deviation at mid-therapy and initial level of functioning. Better-functioning clients tended to view therapists as similar to both mother and father, while clients with greater initial pathology saw the therapist as dissimilar to father and mother. “I. (. 69 Level of personality functioning seemed to be related to the readiness of the client to perceive the therapist as similar to one or both parents. Therapist gender was also clearly a factor. Higher- functioning clients appeared to more readily view the therapist in ways that recapitulated old object relationships, while lower-functioning clients seemed somewhat less likely to do so. The hesitancy of lower-functioning clients could be attributed to a number of factors. It could signal a characteristic guardedness and hesitancy to form relationships, evidenced in this case as a lower level of transference after approxnmately ten therapy sessions. Adternatively, it could indicate that these clients' experiences with past significant others such as parents had been disappointing enough that the therapist would be perceived in a somewhat idealized fashion, dissimilar to either parent, both in emotionally accessibility and in personal strength. Better-functioning clients seemed more capable of forming a therapeutic relationship, while more poorlybfunctioning clients appeared less open to the therapist. These findings are in line with previous clinical reports of therapy outcome from a number of sources. As Strupp (1980) phrased it, By the same token, the more deficient, troubled, and destructive the patient's experience with significant others has been the greater will be the difficulties in achieving a corrective experience in psychotherapy; This is the reason therapy often faces an uphill battle and it defines the limits of what can be achieved in a given period of time. Patients with severely disturbed earlier relationships frequently have great difficulty in forming a good 'therapeutic alliance' with the therapist, a sine qua non for therapeutic change. (p. 291) Schwager and Spear (1981) cited Appelbaum's analysis of the results of the Menninger Foundation Psychotherapy Research Project, 70 which found results parallel to those in this study. In Appelbaum's work, as well as in this study, this 'rich get richer' phenomenon accounted for the greatest improvement subsequent to moderate or long- term psychotherapy among those patients who had shown highest levels of ego functioning at initial testing, and little structural change among patients with more severe initial pathology. Harty, Cerney, Colson, Coyne, Frieswyk, Johnson, and Mortimer also reported similar results in their 1981 study and commented, The strongest finding here thus seems to repeat a familiar one in the field of psychotherapy research: that those who begin a clinical process (here hospital treatment) with greater ego strength, less disorganization, and more accomplishments are very likely to be those who end in the best condition as well...Do these findings, as some have held, cast doubt on the effectiveness of the treatment? we suggest that the answer is yes only when exaggerated or inappropriately 'equalitarian' expectations are made of what treatment should accomplish. In hospital work, as in psychotherapy, psychological processes are to a great extent both the object of the work and the medium in which the work is carried out, i.e., both the material to be shaped and the tools for shaping. we should not be dismayed to find that the resources one brings to such a task determine, to a major extent, what can be accomplished. (p. 221) Some subjects' spontaneous comments on the Helping Relationship Questionnaire confirmed this idea. One wrote, "It is important to note that with any therapist it would probably take a long time for me to really open up. My therapist did a good job, the result is a matter of temporal limitations not quality.” The same client told her therapist during termination that she believed it “would take a bulldozer“ to get through to her and create a meaningful therapeutic relationship marked by trust. Another client offered, 'I believe therapy has been helpful and that my therapist has been trying to get me to open up, but it's been really hard for me to allow myself to open up and express my 71 feeling which in turn is making therapy seem to move or proceed slowly." Maternal/Paternal Aspects and Temporal Changes in Transference It was predicted that the transference would take a predominantly maternal or paternal quality in mid-therapy which would be greatly attenuated at late therapy, as the client worked through termination issues. This would be indicated by the representation of the therapist being significantly more like one parent than the other in mid-therapy, and a significant difference occurring between perceived similarity between the therapist and that parent in mid-therapy, and the therapist and that parent in late therapy. A predominantly maternal or paternal focus was observed in subjects' perceptions of the therapist, although this pattern was not robust enough to be quantitatively confirmed. Six clients saw the therapist as paternal, and nine perceived the therapist in a maternal fashion. No high-functioning clients had paternal transferences to female therapists, but three high-functioning clients had maternal transferences to male therapists. The remaining four high-functioning clients' transferences were Specific to the gender of the therapist. The occurrence of paternal transferences in three clients of female therapists is an anomaly, according to most existing literature. Mid-therapy transference quality took on added significance when late therapy therapist—mother and therapist-father deviations were examined. The significant difference between these deviations disappeared in late therapy. Maternal transferrers in particular seemed to view the therapist as nearly equally similar to each parent in late therapy. Although the predominant maternal or paternal quality 72 seemed to diminish in late therapy, there was not a significant difference between mid- and late therapy. Iate Therapy ICL and Object Relations Scores The conceptual rationale of Hypothesis 7 was that ”moderate" ICL representations indicated a more highly differentiated vieW'of self and others. It was believed that the phenomena of splitting, commonly found in clients with lower levels of ego functioning, might be shown on the ICL by representations of self and others at ”extreme” levels of intensity. Therefore, the number of moderate late-therapy representations was expected to correlate with the capacity for 4; differentiation and mature object relationships, which was believed to be represented in the overall mean Rorschach object relations score. The correlation between these scores was positive but nonsignificant. It seems that, as with Hypothesis 5, the differences in levels of functioning assessed by Rorschach and ICl variables led to low relationships between the two measures. gum... APPENDICES APPENDIX A Therapist Information Form 73 APPENDIX A MEN'S COUNSELINS S‘IUDY Wendy Sabbath, M.A. Introduction This study is actually my doctoral dissertation. I hope to collect the data here at the Counseling Center this year, and this requires the help of the other staff members and interns, in terms of convincing qualified clients to be subjects. All I am asking is that you introduce the study to potential subjects (hopefully in an enthusiastic manner). If they are interested, I will do all the subsequent testing and make the necessary arrangements. Subjects Any female client who is at least 18 years old, is a new client to you, and whom you plan to see for at least six months is eligible. The only other criteria are that the client be only mildly to moderately disturbed, and not currently actively suicidal, or otherwise severely impaired in her ability to make a therapeutic relationship. If you are unsure whether a particular client is eligible, please give her the benefit of the doubt, and later you and I can discuss the issue. Procedure During the first actual therapy hour, I would like you to read or paraphrase as closely as possible the following statement: This year an intern at the Counseling Center is undertaking a study of some issues in women's emotional development, and the ways in which counseling aids these processes. we are asking women to participate in this study. Participation would involve meeting three times with one of our counselors to complete several measures. Ybur identity would be kept strictly anonymous. Ybur decision.whether or not to participate would have no bearing on your counseling, but we would like to encourage you to take part. One of the benefits you would receive would be the opportunity at the end of the study to discuss*with the investigator what the tests suggest about your counseling experience, and.what the experience had been like for you. If at this point the client is willing to participate, or wants more information before deciding, please hand her an Initial Contact Sheet to complete. These should be returned to my mailbox, and I will take it from there. If she is unwilling, please thank her anyway. If she is unsure whether she is interested, please try to persuade her to fill out the Initial Contact Sheet so that I can talk to her directly and respond to her specific concerns. It's hard to know in advance what clients' particular questions or objections might be, but I'm very willing to address these concerns and try to make this as interesting and non-threatening an experience as possible. Most people find taking the tests I am using in this study to be a revealing experience for them, and as mentioned above, I am very willing, after all the testing 11 74 is completed, to go over each subject's tests with them and discuss in general what their tests mean to them. I think this is clear. If not, please let me know and I'll be glad to try to clear things up. I can't do this study without your help, so I'd like to thank you in advance for your assistance, and I'll figure out a way to make my gratitude more tangible later. APPENDIX B Initial Contact Sheet 75 APPENDIX B MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Women's Counseling Study INITIAL CIJNTACI‘ SHEET 1 am interested in getting some more information about the study of women's development and perhaps participating in the study. My name a?" is and my home phone number is The best time to reach me is (If there is another phone number at which you may be reached, please indicate it here, along with the times you may be reached at that number ). I understand that I will be called within the next several days regarding this project. APPENDIX C Consent Form l. 76 APPENDIX C MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Department of Psychology DEPARTMENTAL RESEARCH.CONSENT FORM I have freely consented to take part in a scientific study being conducted by wendy Sabbath, M.A. under the supervision of Professor Bertram Karon at the M.S.U. Counseling Center. This study involves aspects of women's emotional growth and development and investigating the ways in which counseling aids that process. I understand that if I complete the study I wall be contacted three times and that the total time commitment will be less than ten hours over the course of six to nine months. The study has been explained to me and I understand the explanation that has been given and what my participation will involve. I understand that I am free to discontinue my participation in the study at any time without penalty. I understand that the results of the study will be treated in strict confidence and will remain anonymous. ‘Within these restrictions, results of the study will be made available to me at my request. I understand that I may choose to have some of the information made available to my therapist. I understand that my participation in the study does not guarantee any beneficial results to me. I understand that, at my request, I can receive additional explanation of the study after my participation is completed. Signed: Date: APPENDIX D Helping Relationship Questionnaire 77 APPENDIX D dee # The Helping Relationshipgguestionnaire Below are listed a variety of ways that one person may feel or behave in relation to another person. Please consider each statement‘with reference to your present relationship*with your therapist. Mark each statement according to how strongly you feel that it is true, or not true, in this relationship. Please mark every one. ‘write in 6, 5, 4, 3, 2, or 1 to stand for the following answers. 6. Yes, I strongly feel that it is true. 5. Yes, I feel it is true. 4. Yes, I feel that it is probably true, or more true than untrue. 3. No, 2. NO, 1. NO, l. 2. 3. 6. 7. 10. 11. I feel that it is probably untrue, or more untrue than true. I feel it is not true. I strongly feel that it is not true. I believe that my therapist is helping me. I believe that the treatment is helping me. I have obtained some new understanding. I have been feeling better recently. I can already see that I will eventually work out the problems I came to treatment (counseling) for. I feel I can depend upon the therapist. I feel the therapist understands me. I feel the therapist wants me to achieve my goals. I feel I am working together with the therapist in a joint effort. I believe we have similar ideas about the nature of my problems. I feel now that I can understand myself and deal‘with myself on my own (that is, even if the therapist and I were no longer meeting for appointments). Kinds of change: I feel improved in the following ways: I feel worse in the following ways: Estimate of improvement so far: not at all (please circle one) slightly moderately mush very mush Any further comments: REFERENCES Aronow, E. & Reznikoff, M. (1976). Rorschach content interpretation. New York: Grune and Stratton. Athey, C., Fleischer, J., & Coyne, L. (1980). Rorschach object representation as influenced by thought and affect organization. In J. Kvawer, H. Ierner, P. Ierner, & A. Sugarman, Borderline phenomena and the Rorschach test. NeW'YoIk: International Universities Press. Auerbach, A. (1983). Assessment of psychotherapy outcome from the viewpoint of expert observer. In M. Lambert, E. Christensen, & S. De Julio, The assessment of psychotherapy outcome. New'York: wuley. Beier, E. & Ratzeburg, F. (1953). The parental identifications of male and female college students. JOurnal of Abnormal Psychology, 38, 569-572. Blatt, S., Brennais, C., Schimek, J., & Glick, M. (1976). Normal development and psychopathological impairment of the concept of the object on the Rorschach. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 364-373. Blatt, S. & Lerner, H. (1983). The psychological assessment of object representation. JOurnal of Personality Assessment, 21, 7-28. Blotcky, A. (1984). Early use of the Rorschach in anticipating the transference of borderline adolescents. Dynamic Psychotherapy, 2, 157-165. Buckley, P., Conte, H., Plutchik, R.,‘Wild, K., & Karasu, T. (1984). Psychodynamic variables as predictors of psychotherapy outcome. American JOurnal of Psychiatry, 141, 742-748. Cava, E. & Raush, H. (1952). Identification and the adolescent boy's perception of his father. JOurnal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 855-856. Clark, T. & Taulbee, E. (1981). A comprehensive and indexed bibliography of the Interpersonal Check List. JOurnal of Personality Assessment, 25, 505-525. Crowder, J. (1972). Relationship between therapist and client interpersonal behaviors and psychotherapy outcome. JOurnal of Counseling Psychology, 19, 68-75. 78 79 Crowder, J. & Feltoon, H. (1973). Transference: Analysis of the concept and review of the research. San Antonio: Tri-way Printers and Mailers. Dietzel, C. & Abeles, N. (1975). Client-therapist complementarity and therapeutic outcome. JOurnal of CounselingPsychology, 22, 264- 272. Elizur, A. (1949). Content analysis of the Rorschach with regard to anxiety and hostility. Journal of Projective Techniques, l3, 247- 284. Exner, J. (1974). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (Vbl. 1). New'York: JOhn Wiley & Sons. Exner, J. (1978). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (Vbl. 2). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Exner, J. (1986). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (2nd ed.). New York: Jehn Wiley & Sons. Freud, S. (1937). Analysis terminable and interminable. (Standard Edition). London: Hogarth Press, 1964, 216-253. Gill, M. (1982). Analysis of transference (vol. 1). New Yerk: International Universities Press. Gill, M. & Hoffman, I. (1982). A method for studying the analysis of aspects of the patient's experience of the relationship in psychoanalysis and psychotherapy. JOurnal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 30, 137-167. Goldfried, M. (1980). Some views on effective principles of psychotherapy. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 271-306. Graff, H. & Luborsky, L. (1977). Long term trends in transference and resistance: A report on a quantitative-analytic method applied to four psychoanalyses. JOurnal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 25, 471-490. Greenspan, S. & Wieder, S. (1984). Dimensions and levels of the therapeutic process. Psychotherapy, gl, 5-23. Groesbeck, C. (1978). Psychological types in the analysis of the transference. JOurnal of Analytic Psychology, 23, 23-53. Guntrip, H. (1973). Psychoanalytic theory, thergEYr and the self. New York: Basic Books. Harty, M., Cerney, M., Colson, D., Coyne, In, Frieswyk, S., Jehnson, S., & Mortimer, R. (1981). Cerrelates of change and long-term outcome. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 45, 209-228. 80 Hoornaert, F. & Pierloot, R. (1976). Transference aspects of doctor- patient relationship in psychosomatic patients. British JOurnal of Medical Peyehology,‘49, 261-266. Karme', L. (1982). El ana'lisis de un paciente varo'n par una analista: el prdblema de a1 transferencia edipica negativa (The analysis of a male patient by a female analyst: The prdblem of the negative oedipal transference). Revista de Psicoana'lisis, 32, 883-896. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1984, ll, Abstract No. 21067). Kiesler, D. (1983). The 1983 Interpersonal Circle: A taxonomy for complementarity in human transactions. Psychological Review, 29, 185-214. Konig, K. & Tischtau-Schroter, R. (1982). Der interktionelle Anteil der Ubertragung bei Partnerwahl und Partnerveranderung (The interactional part of transference by the choice of partners). Zeitschrift fur Psychosomatische Medizin und Psychoanalyse, 28, 266-279. (From Psychological Abstracts, 1982, 62, Abstract No. 10838). Krieger, M. & Worchel, P. (1960). A test of the psychoanalytic theory of identification. Journal of Individual Psycholegy,‘l§, 56-63. Krohn, A. & Mayman, M. (1974). Object representations in dreams and projective tests. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 38, 445-465. Kulish, N. (1984). The effect of the sex of the analyst on transference. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 28, 95-110. Lapkin, B. & Lury, J. (1983). Transference and countertransference in the light of developmental considerations. American JOurnal of Psychoanalysis, 43, 345-359. Leary, T. (1956). Multilevel measurement of interpersonal behavior: A manual for the use of the interpersonal system of personality. Berkeley, CA.: Psychological Consultation Service. Leary, T. & Harvey, J. (1956). A.methodology for measuring personality changes in psychotherapy. Jburnal of Clinical Lerner, H. (1983). An object representation approach to psychostructural change: A clinical illustration. JOurnal of PersonalityAssessment,.41, 314-323. Lerner, H. & St. Peter, S. (1984). Patterns of object relations in neurotic, borderline and schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry, 42, 77-92 0 81 Lorr, M., McNair, D., Michaux,‘w;, & Raskin, A. (1962). Frequency of treatment and change in psychotherapy. JOurnal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 281-292. Luborsky, L. (1984). Principles of psychoanalytic psychotherepy. New Yerk: Basic Books. Luborsky, L., Crabtree, L., Curtis, H., Ruff, G., & Mintz, J. (1975). The concept 'space' of transference for eight psychoanalysts. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 48, 65-70. McDonald, R. (1965). Ego-control patterns and attribution of hostility to self and others. JOurnal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 273-277. Malcolm, J. (1981). Psychoanalysis: The impossibleprofession. New York: Knopf. Mogul, K. (1982). Overview: The sex of the therapist. American JOurnal of Psychiatry, 139, 1-11. Mueller, W. (1966). Anxiety level, inferred identification and response tendencies on a semantic differential. JOurnal of Counseling Psychology, l}, 144-152. Mueller, W. (1969a). Patterns of behavior and their reciprocal impact in the family and in psychotherapy (Monograph). JOurnal of Counselipg Psychology, lg, (2, pt. 2). Mueller, W; (1969, September). Mapping the psychotherapeutic process through analyeis of interpersonal behavior. Symposium conducted at the American Psychological Association,‘Washington, D.C. Moeller,‘w. & Dilling, C. (1968). Therapist-client interview behavior and personality characteristics of therapists. Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 32, 281-288. Mueller,‘W. 8 Dilling C. (1969). Studying interpersonal themes in psychotherapy research. J0urnal of Counseling Psychology, lg, 50- 58. Rabinowitz, A. (1975). Hostility measurement and its relationship to fantasy capacity. JOurnal of Personality Assessment, 39, 50-54. Rapaport, D., Gill, M., & Schafer, R. (1968). Diagnostic (psychological testing (rev. ed.). New Yerk: International Universities Press. Rawn, M. (1981). A note on unwitting replication: Quantitative studies of transference and resistance twenty years apart. JOurnal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 782-784. 82 Ryan, E. & Bell, M. (1984). Changes in object relations from psychosis to recovery. JOurnal of Abnormal Psychology, 23, 209- 215. Schafer, R. (1977). The interpretation of transference and the conditions for loving. JOurnal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 2;, 335-362. Schlesinger-Vaccaro, R. (1983). A relationship between Piaget's notion of conservation and the transference phenomenon: A clinical example. The Psychoanalytic Review, 19, 83-94. Schrier, H. (1953). The significance of identification in therapy. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 23, 585-604. Schwager, E. & Spear, W; (1981). New perspectives on psychological tests as measures of change. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, fl, 527-541 0 Shapiro, D. A. & Shapiro, D. (1982). Meta-analysis of comparative therapy outcome studies: A replication and refinement. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 581-604. Silverman, In & weinberger, J. (1985). Mommy and I are one: Implications for psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 49, 1296- 1308. Smith, M., Glass, G., & Miller, T. (1980). The benefits of psychotherapy. Baltimore: The Jehns prkins University Press. Sopchak, A. (1952). Parental 'identification' and 'tendency toward disorders’ as measured by the Minnesota MUltiphasic Personality Inventory. JOurnal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 159- 165. Spear, W; & Lapidus, L. (1981). Qualitative differences in manifest object representations. JOurnal of Abnormal Psychology, 29, 157- 167. Tracey, T. & Ray, P. (1984). Stages of successful time-limited counseling: An interactional examination. JOurnal of Cbunselipg Psycholegy, 3_1_, 13-27. Tuber, S. (1983). Children's Rorschach scores as predictors of later adjustment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, §l, 379-385. Urist, J. (1977). The Rorschach test and the assessment of object relations. JOurnal of Personality Assessment, gl, 3-9. 83 Urist, J. & Shill, M. (1982) . Validity of the Rorschach Mutuality of Autonomy Scale: A replication using excerpted responses. Journal of Personality Assessment, 4g, 450-454. Waelder, R. (1956) . Introduction to the discussion on problems of transference. Internatioral Jounal of Psychoanalysis, g, 367- 368. Weiner, M. (1982) . Identification in psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychotherapy, g, 109-116. Zetzel, E. (1956). Current concepts of transference. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, _3_'_7_, 369-376.