.. H... ‘.-.......‘. _. 7 ‘ ’nv .. . 4w.-‘o.. .,........ ' _-‘ —‘ H "U v V V . I ~ . a u ' I V . . - I ‘ . , _ V , ‘ . . . . .. - .. '- p ‘ . . . H V .. . . - a , V . . . . .. . .--.‘- A... ,, . n4- ' -“"’~“ " llllllll lllllllllllllllllllllllll'lll 1293 00700 3902 THESIS LIBRARY Michigan State University -AA~ ' ‘ WV 7“-" "1' This is to certify that the thesis entitled Hemisphericity and Jungian Typology presented by Daniel G. Mosher has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. Jegree in Psychology 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution MSU LIBRARIES "- RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. HEMISPHERICITY AND JUNGIAN TYPOLOGY BY Daniel G. Mosher A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1985 ABSTRACT HEMISPHERICITY AND JUNGIAN TYPOLOGY BY Daniel G. Mosher This study explored hypothesized relationships between hemisphericity and Jungian typology. The Human Information Processing Survey (HIP) and the Singer-Loomis Inventory of Personality (SLIP) were administered to 117 undergraduates, later classified by sex and as "hard" and ”soft” academic majors, who were compared for typological and hemisphericity differences. Despite serious psychometric weaknesses observed in both instruments, the findings partially supported the gen- eral hypothesis of linkages between hemisphericity and Jungian typology. The strongest connections were the predic- ted correlations of Right Hemisphericity with Intuition (E = .32, p<.001) and Thinking (£’= -.30, p<.001). Unexpectedly, however, there were no significant linkages to Left Hemis- phericity. Notable support was found for two basic tenets of Jungian psychology: (a) the bipolarity of the functions of Thinking vs. Feeling (r = -.50) and Sensation vs. Intuition (E = -.44); and (b) typal sex differences (men scored sig- nificantly higher on Extraverted Thinking, Extraverted Sen- sation, Thinking, and thraversion, while women significant- ly exceeded men on Introverted Feeling, Introversion and Feeling). Further improvements in both measures appear crucial for a more definitive appraisal of their linkages. To my sister Sue, with love. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS There are many people that I am indebted to for making this project possible. I would like to give special thanks to my chairperson, Dr. John Hurley, who has been so helpful through all phases of my graduate career. If this project has been a success, much of the credit must go to him. Dr. Joseph Reyher, who has shared my excitement for research, was always there with a word of encouragement and a helpful suggestion. I also want to thank Dr. Elaine Donelson who, through her insightful comments and shared interests, made this work much the better for her influence. Others that I wish to thank include: Dr. Mary Loomis for her very special help; Marty Cosgrow, who was an invalu- able aid in data collection and analysis; Dr. Louis Wilson for our many helpful discussions; Dean Sandell, who was so generous with his graphics expertise; and Sandra DeSantis for her excellent typing. And finally, I wish to thank my wife, Diann, who has been so supportive throughout this whole project. She has shared my excitement and sense of accomplishment, and also my disappointments and fears. Her many sacrifices made this all possible. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Hemisphericity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Jungian Typology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Relationship Between Hemisphericity and Jungian Typology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 Design and Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 Scoring . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ; . . . . . 18 Hemisphericity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 Jungian Typology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 Hemisphericity and Jungian Typology . . . . . . . 25 Sex Differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 Differences in College Major . . . . . . . . . . 35 DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 The Functions and Right Hemisphericity . . . . . 38 The Functions and Left Hemisphericity . . . . . . 41 iv Attit Sex D Colle Other Impli APPENDICES A. REFERENCES ude and Hemisphericity . . . . . . . . ifferences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ge Major . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . cations O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Comparison of Left, Right Brain Dichotomies and Rational/Nonrational Functions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Spearman Correlation Coefficients for HIP Survey and SLIP . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Representative Sample of HIP Left Hemisphere (LH) Items and SLIP Thinking Items . . . . . . . . . 43 44 45 46 47 49 50 51 52 Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. Table 6. Table 7. Table 8. Table 9. Table 10. LIST OF TABLES Means, Standard Deviations and Norms for HIP Survey Subscale Scores . . . . . . . Spearman Correlation Coefficients for HIP Survey and SLIP Attitude and Function scores 0 O O I O O O O O O O O O O I O I O 0 Classification of Participants According to Hemispheric Preference . . . . . . . . . Means and Standard Deviations for SLIP Subscale Scores (N = 117) . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Mean Hemisphericity Scores Between Thinkers and Intuitives (df = 52). . Comparison of Mean Function and Cognitive Mode Scores Between Left and Right Dominant Hemispheric Individuals (df = 44) . . . . . A Comparison of Mean Hemisphericity Scores Between Introverted Nonrationals and Extra- verted Rationals (df = 47) . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance Expressing the Relationship Between Sex and Hemisphericity for Sensation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Mean Differences Between Males and Females for Hemisphericity, Attitude, Function, and Cognitive Mode Scores (df = 115) . . . . Mean Differences Between Hard and Soft College Majors for Hemisphericity, Attitude, Function, and Cognitive Mode Scores (df = 106) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi PAGE 19 20 22 23 28 30 31 33 34 36 LIST OF FIGURES PAGE Figure 1. Covariance of Significantly Associated HIP Survey (LH & RH) and SLIP (T, F, S, & N) Measures 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 42 vii INTRODUCTION Hemisphericity, a development of recent trends in brain research (Springer & Deutsch, 1981), and Jungian typology (Jung, 1971) are both theories of human information process- ing, or cognitive style. Each deals with the receiving and processing of information within the individual. In addition to their functional similarity, the constructs of hemispher- icity and Jungian typology appear very similar from a theoretical and definitional standpoint (Rossi, 1977; Stevens, 1983). If this can be verified empirically, it would further validate both theories and expand their poten- tial application. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between these seemingly separate and distinct though conceptually similar, measures of cognitive style. Hemisphericity Asymmetries in hemispheric function were first discov- ered in the nineteenth century through the study of the differential effects of injury to the left and right halves of the human brain. Interest in this topic increased dramatically after the split-brain operations of the 1960's and led to a rapid growth in research seeking to character- 1 2 ize these differences and their implications for human behavior (Sprinter & Deutsch, 1981). As a result of this expanding interest in brain hemis- phericity, there is considerable documentation associating the left hemisphere in most individuals with verbal, sequen- tial, and analytic modes of cognition, and the right hemis— phere with visual, spatial, intuitive, and synthetic modes (Galin & Ornstein, 1972; Sperry, 1974; Springer & Deutsch, 1981). An exception to this pattern can be seen in left handers, about 15 percent of whom show right-hemisphere control of speech and another 15 percent who show bilateral control (Springer & Deutsch, 1981). There are also data indicating that it is possible to identify individual differences in the tendency to rely more on one cerebral hemisphere than upon the other in processing information (Bever, 1975; Bogen, Dezure, Tenhouten & Marsh, 1972; Ornstein, 1972). While not all studies have confirmed this (Arndt & Berger, 1979), Kinsbourne (1982) concluded that there was some division of labor between the hemis- pheres and that attempts to capture the essence of this dichotomy in a simplifying formulation, such as hemispher- icity, are justifiable. The most frequently used methods of assessing hemis- phericity have relied on lateral eye movements (LEMs) and dichotic listening. But there have been problems with both methods. There are concerns that dichotic listening typical- ly underestimates the incidence of left-hemispheric Speech 3 in right handers, that strategy may contribute to perfor- mance, and that reliability has been lower than expected (Springer & Deutsch, 1981). Similarly, in a review of the evidence linking LEMs to hemispheric asymmetry, Ehrlichman & Weinberger (1979) noted that the relationship is weak and indirect, based primarily on the investigator’s conceptions of what constitutes a left or right-hemispheric question. As an alternative, Torrance and his co-workers (Torrance, Reynolds, Riegel, & Bell, 1977) developed a self- report instrument to assess hemisphericity or style of processing information. Originally called Your Style of Learning and Thinking (SOLAT), this instrument is now known as the Human Information Processing Survey (HIP Survey) and was selected for use in this study. There is a growing body of literature documenting the HIP's reliability and validity (Taggart & Torrance, 1984). Jungian Typology According to Jung's theory of psychological types (Jung, 1971), there are two opposite but complementary attitudes, introversion and extraversion. The extraverted attitude is an outward focusing of attention, or libido, toward objects, people and the environment. The introverted attitude, on the other hand, is an inward turning of the libido toward the subject. Introverts attend more to ideas, concepts, impressions, feelings, and images. Davis and Johnson (1983) sum up the differences well: Introverts are oriented predominantly to the subject as the basis of their consciousness, while 4 extraverts are oriented more to the object as the basis of their consciousness. . . . For the intro- vert, experience always appears mediated by his experiencing of it. . . . What the extravert experiences at any given moment is felt to be more directed by the sights or sounds themselves. . . . The distinction between extraverts and introverts must be viewed from the perspective of differing perceptions or ways of processing information. It is not that introverts do not like people or with- draw. What introverts do with information result- ing from interactions with others distinguish them from extraverts (p. 151). In their study, the authors found that when the con- tents of subjects' consciousness were assessed using a paging device, the data showed that introverts were more likely to make reference to words denoting processing (e.g., awareness, impressions, image, experience, etc.). Extra- verts, on the other hand, were more likely to make reference to time and objects. These findings were consistent with Jung's distinctions between introverts and extraverts. In addition to the two basic attitudes of introversion and extraversion, there are two pairs of functional types. One pair (sensation and intuition) receives information and the other pair (thinking and feeling) processes and evalu- ates that information. It is these two processes perception and evaluation) that relate Jung's system to cognitive information processing theory besides being a theory of personality. Sensation and intuition are modes of perception. They are irrational or nonrational functions in that they act solely as receivers of information. They do not evaluate, interpret or judge. Sensation Operates through the five 5 senses and is tangible and concrete, whereas intuition is perception via unconscious means or processes. Thinking and feeling are the reflective or judging functions. They are rational functions in that they are methods of evaluating information acquired through one of the modes of perception. In the case of thinking, the ques- tion is whether something is valid or invalid, reasonable or unreasonable, logical or illogical. On the other hand, feel- ing judgments are not concerned with validity but with whether something is important or unimportant, valuable or worthless. They are a personal statement of values. It is important to note that Jung used the term feeling to mean value judgments, not affect. This is very similar to the class of feelings or ”affective judgments" that Zajonc (1980) refers to in taking a position similar to Jung on the independence of feeling and thinking. Jung's theory of psychological types was based on the assumption that thinking and feeling were bipolar Opposites, as were sensation and intuition. According to this bipolar- ity assumption, the more highly developed one of the func- tion pairs became, the less developed would be the OppOSite. Thus, if one's dominant functional type is rational (e.g., thinking) then one's inferior (least developed) function should tend to be the opposite--in this case, nonrational (e.g., sensation). The two most widely used measures of psychological type are the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Myers, 1962) and the \ 6 Gray-Wheelwrights Jungian Type Survey (Wheelwright, Wheel- wright & Buehler, 1964). The former has received consider- able study and some validation (Metzner, 1980; Myers, 1980), and the two tests correlate highly with each other (Myers, 1962). Both instruments use a forced-choice format based on Jung's bipolarity assumption. This means that one must choose between thinking and feeling items and between sensa- tion and intuition items. For example, as one selects more thinking items, the number of feeling items is necessarily less. This prevents thinking and feeling, or sensation and intuition from appearing as the two most highly developed functions and builds in a negative correlation between the paired functions. One study found that when scaled items replaced forced- choice items, 61% of the subjects scored a different superior function and 48% did not have an inferior (least developed) function that was the hypothesized opposite of their superior function (Loomis & Singer, 1980). These results did not prove that Jung's observations of opposing tendencies between function pairs was incorrect. They suggested, rather, that it is not impossible to transcend the bipolar opposites and that in some personality profiles the functions may be independent. Consequently, several investigators (Loomis, 1982; Metzner, 1980; Metzner, Burney & Mahlberg, 1981) have pointed out the need for a new perspective for viewing and measuring Jung's typology. As a result, the Singer-Loomis Inventory of Personality (SLIP) 7 was deveIOped as an independent measure of psychological type (Singer & Loomis, 1984) and was selected for use in the present study. Relationship Between Hemisphericity and Jungian Typology Jung's rational and nonrational functional types appear to be closely related to hemisphericity. Both approaches to cognitive style are based on dichotomies. When Jung's (1971) dichotomies for describing these two functional types were compared with dichotomies describing left, right brain func- tioning (Bogen, 1969; Springer et a1., 1981), there was a very close match between the rational type and the left brain and the nonrational type and the right brain (see Appendix A). Both the rational type and the left brain were described as intellectual, thinking, rational, objective and directed. The nonrational type and the right brain, on the other hand, were described using terms such as sensation, intuition, perception and existential. Furthermore, in a discussion of recent psychological research on hemispheric functioning, Rossi (1977) also assigned Jung's attitude of extraversion to the left hemis- phere and introversion to the right. He went on to state: Traditional scholarship strongly documents the view that the right side of the body (controlled by the left hemisphere) is associated with extra- version, the dexterous interaction with the exter- nal world, while the left side of the body (right hemisphere controlled) by contrast is associated with the sinister, the dark and introverted (Jung, 1971). ' Jung's rational functions of thinking and feeling are evidently left hemispheric while the irrational functions of sensation and intuition 8 are more closely associated with the right hemis- pheric functioning. Thinking and feeling [value judgments] are both rational functions that require the use of the verbal and logical func- tions of the left hemisphere. The irrational func- tions of sensation and intuition, on the other hand, are more perceptual and thus more closely related to the synthetic functioning of the right For the most part, Stevens (1983) agreed with Rossi's designations, except Stevens separated the feeling function into two components: an affectual component localized in the right hemisphere and an evaluative or judgment component in the left. Rossi (1977) also pointed out that there is some empir- ical support for the relationship between hemisphericity and Jungian typology. The relationship of the nonrational func- tion of intuition to the right hemisphere is supported by a study in which Nebes (1974) found that the right hemisphere excelled "in the performance of at least one cognitive Operation--generating from partial or fragmented information a percept of the whole stimulus” (p. 163). This ability to synthesize the whole from the part could be the basic process underlying Jung's definition of intuition as one of the basic functions of the psyche, namely, the perception of the possibilities inherent in a situation (Jung, 1971). Additional evidence for the link between hemisphericity and Jung's typology comes from Fischer's (1975) research on consciousness. He found that 90% of his college student subjects who volunteered for experiments to explore ”inner space” scored as introverts with intuition as their dominant 9 function (INFP) on the Meyers-Briggs Type Indicator. Fischer stated that "Given our present state of knowledge, the INFP score seems to denote volunteers with preferen- tially right-hemispheric involvements and interests, whereas high scores on the extraversion (A), down-to-earth sensation (S), thinking (T), and judging (J) scales [extraverts with thinking as dominant function] on the MBTI point to left- hemispheric predominance (called secondary process by Freud)" (p. 218). Hypotheses On the basis of the definitional, theoretical, and empirical considerations discussed above, the following hypotheses were tested: Hypothesis I: Introversion and the nonrational functions (sensation and intuition) should be positively correlated with right hemispheric information processing and negatively correlated with the left hemispheric processing. In addition, extraversion and the rational functions (thinking and feeling) should be positively correlated with the left hemisphere and negatively with the right. Hypothesis II: Rational individuals (those classified as thinkers or feelers) should have higher mean scores for left hemisphericity than nonrational individuals (those classified as sensates or intuitives) and that nonrationals should have higher right hemisphere scores than rationals. Hypothesis III: Left hemisphere dominant individuals should have higher rational function and cognitive mode scores and right-dominant individuals should score higher on nonrational functions and cognitive modes. 10 Hypothesis IV: When attitudes and functions are combined, introverts with a dominant nonrational function (sensation or intuition) should score significantly higher on right hemisphericity than on left hemisphericity. Conversely, extraverts with a dominant rational function (thinking or feeling) should score higher on left hemispher- icity than right. In addition to these primary hypotheses, this study also examined the relationship between hemisphericity and Jungian typology and subjects' sex and choice of college major. There is some evidence to suggest that males tend to be more lateralized for verbal and spatial abilities and women show greater bilateral representation for both types of functions (Springer & Deutsch, 1981). If this is the case, there may be sex differences in hemisphericity scores. And both hemisphericity and Jung's typology have been related to choice of college major (Bakan, 1969; Myers, 1980). This study attempted to replicate Bakan's (1969) findings that right hemisphere dominant individuals were more likely to choose "soft" college majors and left-handed dominant individuals more likely to select ”hard" college majors. METHOD Participants Participants in this study consisted of 117 college students from an introductory psychology course. The group was made up of 57 males and 60 females. Each person received supplemental credit toward course grades for participating. Materials The instruments used in this research are described below: 1. The Singer-Loomis Inventory of Personality (SLIP). The SLIP is the newest of the instruments based on Jung's typology (Jung, 1971). It provides scores for each of eight functional types or cognitive modes based on Jung's two attitudes of Intro- version and Extraversion and the four functions of Thinking, Feeling, Sensation, and Intuition (Singer & Loomis, 1984). The relative development of the eight cognitive modes within an individual represents that person's cognitive style. The SLIP offers a major improvement over other Jungian instruments such as the Gray-Wheelwright (Wheel- wright et al., 1964) and the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (Myers, 1962) in that it uses scaled ll 12 items rather than a forced-choice format as noted above. This lack of constraint allows Opposite functions such as Thinking and Feeling to be measured independently, whereas with the earlier instruments the choice of one of these bipolar opposites automatically resulted in the rejection of the other. Using equivalent forms, reliability coefficients ranged from .73 to .80 for the four functions and were .85 and .83 for Introversion and thraversion, respectively. The internal organization of the SLIP items was examined by factor analysis (Singer & Loomis, 1984). In the principal component analysis, two factors emerged that were rational (judging) factors and two other factors emerged that were nonrational (perceptual) factors. The first factor, labeled Judging (Reflective), accounted for 7.70% of the variance. The second factor accounted for 6.98% of the variance and was labeled Judging (Active). Factor 3 accounted for 6.86% of the variance. This factor was labeled Perceptual based on the item content. Factor 4, labeled Perceptual (Affective), accounted for 4.73% of the variance. The four factors that emerged were in accord with Jung's theory and gave indirect support to the construct validity of the four functions; two judging/ reflective (Thinking and Feeling) functions and 13 two perceptual (Sensation and Intuition) func- tions. The present version of the SLIP was then developed by evaluating each item on the basis of this and several other factor analyses and rewriting those items with low or imcompatible factor loadings. The predictive validity of the SLIP was examined in a study investigating the relationship between cognitive styles and artistic styles (Loomis & Saltz, 1984). As predicted, Extraverted artists produced more figurative, representational art, whereas Introverted artists produced more nonfigurative, abstract art. The study also found predicted differences in style between artists with Rational and Nonrational cognitive styles. Rational artists tended to produce art that incorporated recognizable elements and was arranged in conformance to rational expectations. Nonrational artists, on the other hand, were likely to produce art which incorporated unusual elements and was arranged unpredictably. In sum, while the SLIP appears to be organized along interrelated dimensions consis- tent with Jung's typology, evidence supporting its validity is limited, based primarily on a single predictive study (Loomis & Saltz, 1984). Human Information Processing Survey (HIP Survey). The HIP Survey (formerly Your Style of Learning 14 and Thinking--SOLAT) was develOped by Torrance et a1. (1977). It consists of 40 items and measures Right hemisphere (RH) specialization, Left hemis- phere (LH) specialization and integrative (IH) style using a multiple-choice format. Normative data from Taggart and Torrance (1984) are shown in Table 1. Since its development, there has been an increasing number of studies supporting the con- struct validity of the HIP Survey. Many of these studies have been reviewed and summarized by Taggart and Torrance (1984). A number of research- ers using the present instrument and its predeces- sors (Forms A and B of SOLAT) have explored the problem of construct validity by administering the instrument to occupational or disciplinary groups hypothesized to have a particular preferred infor- mation processing style. One of the largest of these studies was conducted by Schwartz and DeMattei (1982) with responses from 15,470 ani magazine readers. They found that RH scores were associated with artistic occupations. The category of occupations that included psychologists, home- makers and athletic coaches also tended to score higher on RH. Occupations involving the investiga- tion of physical, biological and cultural phenom- ena tended to score high on LH. In a similar study, Ghosh (1980) found that mathematically 15 gifted students preferred LH processing, and the musically and artistically gifted showed a very distinct RH preference. While studies such as these are numerous, this "bootstrapping" approach to validity is weak from a psychometric standpoint and the validity of the HIP survey remains to be clearly demonstrated. Design and Procedure The participants were informed that this study con- cerned cognitive styles and that they would be asked to com- plete two psychological scales. In an effort to minimize demand characteristics and subject roles (Kazdin, 1980), the participants were assured that any information disclosed would be held confidential and that there were no right or wrong answers to the items of these inventories. The HIP Survey and the SLIP were then administered along with detailed instructions on how to complete the instruments. The order in which the instruments were admin- istered was counterbalanced to control for any possible order effects. Participant's sex and choice of college major was also recorded. A trial run was conducted, with ten par- ticipants, in an effort to discover potential problems before the main study was carried out. No difficulties were encountered. Scoring The HIP Survey raw scores for LH, RH, and IH were con- verted to standard scores using the conversion table pro- 16 vided in the HIP manual. The standard score was calculated so that the mean value equaled 100 and the standard devia- tion equaled 20. An individual was identified as LH- dominant, RH-dominant, or IH-dominant if the corresponding standard score was equal to or greater than 120. If no stan- dard scores equaled or exceeded 120, the person was clas- sified as Mixed (MH). The SLIP provided scores for each of the eight cog- nitive modes by allowing participants to score each item on a scale Of 1 to 5 (1 = never, 2 = occasionally, 3 = about half the time, 4 = usually, and 5 = always). Raw scores can be misleading for the SLIP because of response sets. For example, one individual may consistently use only the high end of the scale (e.g., 2'5, 3'5, 4'5) and another individ- ual may use only the low end (e.g., 0'3, 1'8, 2'5). The latter individual would appear less developed than the former for a given cognitive mode due solely to style of response. For this reason the raw scores were converted to percent scores. Each scale's raw score was divided by the total of the raw scores for all eight scales to get a per- cent score. Thus the percent score for each scale indicates the relative strength of the individual's cognitive modes. Scores for the four functions were computed by summing the two corresponding cognitive modes. Thus, the Thinking score was derived by summing the Introverted Thinking and Extraverted Thinking scores. The same procedure determined total scores for Feeling, Sensation, and Intuition. A score 17 for Introversion was calculated by summing the four Intro- verted cognitive modes. Similarly, a score for Extraversion was computed by summing the four extraverted cognitive modes. An individual was classified as Introverted or Extra- verted on the basis of which of the two scores was higher. Individuals were similarly classified as Thinkers, Feelers, Sensates, or Intuitives on the basis of their highest func- tion score. Participants were also classified as ”hard" major, "soft" major, or no preference on the basis of their repor- ted college major. An attempt was made to use a clas- sification system as similar to Bakan's (1969) as possible. For purposes of this analysis ”hard" majors included math- ematics, biology, engineering, economics, physics, account- ing and pre-med. "Soft" majors included psychology, polit- ical science, English, history, communication, advertising, business and pre-law. RESULTS Hemisphericity Means, standard deviations and normative data for the HIP Survey are shown in Table 1. Participants in the present study had significantly higher mean raw scores for LH (t = 2.58 [439], p<.01) but significantly lower mean scores for IH (t = -4.07 [439], p<.001) than the norms from Taggart and Torrance (1984). These significant t-values may reflect real differences in the two populations. The population sample for the present study consisted of undergraduate college students, whereas the norms from Taggart and Torrance came from a general sample of adults. Therefore, the HIP Survey scores from the present study may not be directly comparable to the existing norms. The intercorrelations between the three HIP scales are shown in Table 2. The highly significant negative correla- tions (p<.001) between RH, LH, and IH were a result of the HIP Survey's forced-choice character. For example, as one chose more LH answers, the number of RH and IH choices was necessarily less; thus the -.47 average intercorrelation. This means that on the average, 22% of each measure's var- iance is confounded with each of the other measures. There- fore, in its present format, the HIP Survey yields somewhat 18 19 .vmma .mocmuuoe a uncomma eouu coxmu mump o>wume Hoo.vm«t Hoovmt uoz ”muoz «ano.vu m.m m.mH me.>H mn.am 45.4 mH.¢H omumoomocH om.a o.m N.NH vv.ma ow.¢oa mn.v mo.mH unmfim emm.m ¢.¢ m.HH ww.ma mo.voa m>.v mn.~a puma I am coo: am now: am coo: mamom u mmuoom 3mm mmuoom Oumccmum mmuoom 3mm Avmm u z. Anaa u z. moocmuwmwfla wEuoz Npsum ucommum mouoom mHmOmnom >o>usm mH: How mEuoz cam mcoflumfl>oo Oumpcoum .mcmoz .a canoe 20 .M now Ho. xn >Hmfluflse “Oouuflso coon o>mn mamefiooo "muoz mo.vmo Ho.vm Hoo.vmm . mean can moon Ha: Ha Oman mmm mu cowuflsuca mean . mmmu ca mu m NH new- o coflummcmm 0H- mmm- . mom- e- A SH. cad ma- mcflflmmm mom- 0H mom- . oom oomu nqm mom: 4 mcaxcflza HH- m- 5. com . coal 5 N. a- coflmum>muuxm HH m n oomu coal . n. N v cofimum>ouucH mad: NH Han new 5 ml . oven mvmn wuHOwnocmmHEom coumumoucH mmm nvmu omH mom- m- m was- . mac- sufloflumgamflsmm unmam m. 0 man 4 a: v mam: mavu . wuaoflumndmaemm been oasocH meow Home xcflna m H :H mm mg cam mosuflooa qum cam >m>osm aHm not mucmflofimumoo wQH MOW COHUUCDm coflumaouuoo coauoomm .N magma 21 limited data and this qualification needs to be considered when evaluating the present results. Table 3 shows the classification of participants according to hemispheric preference based on a standard score of 120 as described earlier. As indicated, a majority (55%) did not demonstrate a distinct preference and were classified as Mixed Hemisphere (MH). The finding appears supportive of Levy's (1985) contention that we all depend on intimate collaboration between hemispheres and the popular myth that individuals are "left brained" or "right brained" is based on a misinterpretation of the facts. Jungian Typology Table 4 contains means and standard deviations for SLIP raw and percent scores. The intercorrelations between SLIP attitude and function scales are presented in Table 2. Intercorrelations for the cognitive mode scales are shown in Appendix B. Because the SLIP is an ordinal scale, nonpara- metric Spearman correlation coefficients were used as sug- gested by Siegel (1956) even though histograms of SLIP per- cent scores showed reasonable approximation to normality. The -1 correlation between SLIP's Introversion and Extraversion attitudinal indices was a result of the manner in which the two scores were derived. Employing the present percentage transformation, each individual's Introversion and Extraversion scores sum to 100 percent because they were computed by summing all eight cognitive mode percent scores 22 Table 3. Classification of Participants According to Hemispheric Preference Hemisphere Number Percent Left 26 22 Right 20 17 Integrated 7 6 Mixed _§4 _55 Totals 117 100 23 Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations for SLIP Subscale Scores (N = 117) Raw Scores Percent Scores Scale Mean SD Mean SD Introversion 194.51 21.10 52.85 1.77 Extraversion 173.74 19.80 47.22 1.86 Thinking 93.71 11.55 25.48 1.92 Feeling 92.04 10.48 25.00 1.67 Sensation 91.01 10.62 24.75 1.33 Intuition 91.45 13.06 24.84 1.90 Introverted Thinking 49.79 6.71 13.55 1.26 Introverted Feeling 47.63 5.89 12.93 1.21 Introverted Sensation 49.45 6.03 13.43 .92 Introverted Intuition 47.64 7.27 12.94 1.23 Extraverted Thinking 43.92 5.84 11.93 1.16 Extraverted Feeling 44.45 5.99 12.07 1.11 Extraverted Sensation 41.56 5.57 11.33 .96 Extraverted Intuition 43.81 6.83 11.90 1.13 24 as described earlier. Thus, if one's Introversion score is 60, their Extraversion score must be 40. The intercorrelations between SLIP's four functions (see Table 2) are not artifactual in the sense that each response was free to covary with the others; there were no forced-choices. For example, because each Thinking item was rated independently, Thinking and Feeling had no built-in relationship. Therefore, the significant (p<.001) negative correlations between Thinking and Feeling (-.50) and between Intuition and Sensation (-.44) support Jung's (1971) assump- tion that Thinking and Feeling, like Intuition and Sensa- tion, were bipolar Opposites. The unusual pattern of virtually all negative inter- function correlations in Table 2, with an average r = -.39, appears the product of three major factors: the bipolarity assumption just discussed, the Rational/Nonrational function hypothesis, and SLIP's internal inconsistencies and weak- nesses. The present work hypothesized that Thinking and Feel- ing were Rational functions and would therefore be expected to be positively correlated. But apparently bipolarity was a much stronger influence as evidenced by the resulting neg- ative correlation. Similarly, Sensation and Intuition were hypothesized to be Nonrational functions and positively cor- related. But again bipolarity seems to have been the stronger relationship as reflected by the negative correla- tion. The Rational/Nonrational hypothesis, however, does 25 appear to be partially expressed in the findings as indica- ted by the negative correlation (-.60, p<.001) between Thinking and Intuition and the negative correlation (-.33, p<.001) between Feeling and Sensation. Absent, however, were the anticipated negative correlations between Thinking and Sensation and between Feeling and Intuition. SLIP internal inconsistencies and flaws may also have contributed to this unusual pattern of interfunction rela— tionships. These inconsistencies were evident in the inter- correlations between the eight cognitive modes (see Appendix B). For example, Introverted Thinking is positively correlated (.31, p<.001) with Extraverted Thinking, but there was little correlation (.05) between Introverted Feeling and Extraverted Feeling and also between Introverted Sensation and Extraverted Sensation (.05). Furthermore, SLIP appears heavily biased towards Introversion. When partic- ipants were classified as Introverts or Extraverts on the basis of their Introversion and Extraversion scores, 108 were classified as Introverts, whereas only 6 were Extra- verts and 3 had tie scores. The major point is that SLIP flaws may have affected not only the measured relationships between the functions, but also those found between attitude, function and hemisphericity. Hemisphericity and Jungian Typology Hypothesis I stated that Introversion would be pos- itively correlated with RH and Extraversion positively cor- related with LH and stands rejected. Table 2 showed only 26 trivial Spearman correlations between HIP measures and SLIP's Introversion and Extraversion scales. Hypothesis I also predicted that the Nonrational func- tions (Sensation and Intuition) would correlate positively with RH but negatively with LH and received a mixture of support and refutation. The prediction that the Rational functions (Thinking and Feeling) would correlate positively with LH but negatively with RH also received partial sup- port. Table 2 showed that as predicted, RH was positively correlated (.32, p<.001) with Intuition and negatively cor- related (-.30, p<.001) with Thinking. Although statistically significant, these correlations accounted for only about 10% of the variance in the relationships. Contrary to Hypothesis 1, however, Feeling correlated weakly and positively (.19, p<.05), but Sensation negatively (-.24, p<.01) with RH. It was suspected that the bipolar relationship between the two Nonrational functions (Intuition and Sensation) and between the two Rational functions (Thinking and Feeling) may have created a spurious relationship between RH and the Feeling and Sensation functions. To test this possibility, a partial-correlation between Sensation and RH was computed while controlling for the effects of Intuition. Similarly, a partial-correlation between Feeling and RH was computed while controlling for Thinking. The resulting partial- correlations were .08 and .09, respectively, neither of which was significant at p<.05. These findings suggest that the correlations between Sensation and RH and between 27 Feeling and RH may have been a spurious result of the strong interfunction bipolar relationship. No function correlated significantly with LH (see Table 2) in the manner predicted in Hypothesis I. Although no predictions were made concerning IH, it linked negatively to both Thinking and Sensation (p<.01 and p<.05, respectively). Hypothesis II predicted that persons classified as Rational (SLIP Thinkers or Feelers) would have higher mean LH scores than persons classified as Nonrational (SLIP Sensates or Intuitives) and that Nonrationals would have higher RH scores than Rationals. Hypothesis II was partially supported. Eight one-tailed E-tests were conducted to com- pare mean RH and LH scores between Thinkers (N = 32) and Sensates (N = 8), Thinkers and Intuitives (N = 22), Feelers and Sensates (N = 20), and Feelers and Intuitives. Because no predictions were made regarding IH, two-tailed fittests were used to compare these mean differences. Table 5 shows that as predicted, Intuitives had significantly (p<.01) higher mean RH scores than Thinkers. In addition, Feelers had higher mean RH scores than Sensates (N.= 110.00 & 89.75, §Q = 21.85 and 15.57, respectively). Although the E-value was large enough (3 = 2.38[26], p<.025, one-tailed), these results could not be considered significant because of the one-tailed test, apparently used erroneously. No other group comparisons resulted in significant mean differences. It was expected, as stated in Hypothesis II, that LH- dominant individuals would have higher Rational function and 28 Table 5. Comparison of Mean Hemisphericity Scores Between Thinkers and Intuitives (df = 52) Thinkers Intuitives Differences N = 32 N = 22 Hemisphere Mean SD Mean SD 3 Left 106.19 20.66 99.96 21.54 1.07 Right 96.69 16.35 107.73 13.59 -2.61* Integrated 96.70 15.73 93.00 20.64 .74 *p<.01, one-tailed 29 related cognitive mode scores, while RH-dominant individuals would score higher on Nonrational functions and associated cognitive modes. This hypothesis received partial support. In general, LH-dominant individuals tended to have slightly higher mean scores for Thinking and Sensation functions and cognitive modes (see Table 6), whereas RH-dominant individ- uals tended to have somewhat higher mean scores for Intu- ition and Feeling functions and cognitive modes. However, only the mean differences for Introverted Thinking and Introverted Intuition reached significance (p<.025, one- tailed and p<.001, one-tailed, respectively). The modest tendency for LH-dominant individuals to have higher mean scores for Sensation and RH-dominant individuals to have higher mean scores for Feeling was contrary to expectations. Hypothesis IV stated that when attitude and function were combined, Introverts with a dominant Nonrational func- tion (Sensation or Intuition) would score higher on RH than Extraverts with a dominant Rational function (Thinking or Feeling). This hypothesis was supported (see Table 7). Con- versely, it was predicted that Extraverts with a dominant Rational function would have higher mean scores for LH than Introverted Nonrationals. As Table 7 shows, these results were not significant (p<.05), although the mean difference was in the predicted direction. Sex Differences Two-way analyses of variance were used to test for interactions between sex and hemisphericity with attitude, 30 Table 6. Comparison of Mean Function and Cognitive Mode Scores Between Left and Right Dominant Hemispheric Individuals (df = 44) Left-Dominant Right-Dominant Individuals Individuals N=26 N=20 Function/Cognitive Mode Mean SD Mean SD 1}; Thinking 25.15 1.85 24.75 2.02 .71 Feeling 25.08 1.62 25.45 1.64 -.77 Sensation 24.85 1.32 24.65 1.39 .49 Intuition 24.88 1.86 25.35 1.73 -.87 Introverted Thinking 13.38 .94 12.75 1.21 2.00* Introverted Feeling 12.92 1.20 13.15 1.14 -.65 Introverted Sensation 13.58 .95 13.35 .99 .79 Introverted Intuition 12.86 1.11 13.65 .99 -2.43** Extraverted Thinking 11.77 1.27 12.00 1.21 -.62 Extraverted Feeling 12.15 1.19 12.30 1.08 -.43 Extraverted Sensation 11.27 1.04 11.30 1.13 -.10 Extraverted Intuition 12.00 1.13 11.70 1.30 .84 *p<.025, one-tailed **p<.001, one-tailed 31 Table 7. A Comparison of Mean Hemisphericity Scores Between Introverted Nonrationals and Extraverted Rationals (df = 47) Introverted Nonrationals Extraverted Rationals N = 44 N = 5 Hemisphere Mean SD Mean SD L Left 105.34 19.47 108.20 25.69 -.30 Right 105.75 13.99 89.00 14.80 2.52* Integrated 89.98 19.41 102.60 24.89 -1.34 *p<.01, one-tailed 32 function and cognitive mode as dependent variables; between sex and function with hemisphericity as the dependent var- iables; and between sex and attitude with hemisphericity as the dependent variables. Table 8 shows a significant (p<.05) interaction between sex and hemisphericity for the dependent variable Sensation. An examination of simple effects revealed that IH classified females had significant- ly (E = 4.10[1], p<.05) higher mean Sensation scores than IH males. The means and standard deviations were 26.00(.82) and 24.00(1.00), respectively, for the two groups. On the other hand, Mixed Hemisphere (MH) classified males had signif- icantly (E = 5.84[l], p<.025) higher mean Sensation scores than MH females. The respective means and standard devia- tions for the two groups were 25.09(1.28) and 24.31(1.31). No other statistically significant interactions were found. Table 9 summarizes the mean differences yielded by 17 two-tailed t-tests performed to test for main effects between males and females for hemisphericity, attitude, function and cognitive mode. As indicated in Table 9, males had significantly higher scores for Extraversion, Thinking, Extraverted Thinking and Extraverted Sensation. Females, on the other hand, scored significantly higher on Introversion, Feeling, and Introverted Feeling. Although no specific hypotheses had been formulated regarding sex differences, these results appear to fit the stereotypical descriptions of males as being more Extraverted and Thinking oriented, while females have often been described as tending to be 33 Table 8. Analysis of Variance Expressing the Relationship Between Sex and Hemisphericity for Sensation Source of Variation SS df MS F Sex (A) 8.15 1 8.15 4.87* Hemisphericity (B) 1.98 3 .66 .39 A x B 13.82 3 4.61 2.76* Error 182.19 109 1.67 *E<'05 34 Table 9. Mean Differences Between Males and Females for Hemisphericity, Attitude, Function, and Cognitive Mode Scores (df = 115) Nglgg Females N 57 N = 60 Hemisphericity/ Attitude/Function/ Cognitive Mode Mean SD Mean SD 4; Integrated Hemisphere 92.67 17.65 90.90 17.45 .54 Extraversion 47.98 1.88 46.50 1.53 4.64*** Thinking 25.89 2.02 25.08 1.75 2.32* Sensation 25.02 1.36 24.50 1.27 2.13* Extraverted Thinking 12.32 1.24 11.57 .96 3.66*** Introverted Thinking 13.58 1.34 13.52 1.12 .27 Extraverted Sensation 11.68 .97 10.98 .83 4.21*** Extraverted Feeling 12.10 1.17 12.05 1.06 .18 Left Hemisphere 103.75 20.48 104.33 19.38 -.16 Right Hemisphere 103.77 19.13 105.38 18.98 -.46 Introversion 52.26 1.82 53.40 1.54 -3.65*** Feeling 24.53 1.62 25.45 1.61 -3.10** Intuition 24.81 2.12 24.87 1.68 -.17 Introverted Feeling 12.44 1.15 13.40 1.08 -4.67*** Introverted Sensation 13.33 .91 13.52 .93 -1.08 Introverted Intuition 12.91 1.48 12.97 1.10 -.23 Extraverted Intuition 11.89 1.10 11.90 1.18 -.03 *p<.025 **p<.01 ***p<.001 35 more Introverted and Feeling oriented. There were no signif- icant (p<.05) sex differences found for hemisphericity. Differences in College Major The present study failed to replicate Bakan's (1969) findings that RH-dominant individuals were more likely to choose ”soft" college majors and LH-dominant individuals more likely to select "hard" college majors. The classifica- tion used in this study resulted in 12 out of 19 RH-dominant individuals having "soft" majors, but only 5 out of 24 LH- dominant individuals had "hard" majors. A 2 x 2 contingency analysis yielded a nonsignificant Chi Square of 1.35. Table 10 summarizes the significant mean differences between "hard" and "soft" majors for hemisphericity, attitude, function, and cognitive mode resulting from 17 two-tailed E-tests. Because of the exploratory nature of this aspect of the study, p<.10 was used to test for signif- icance. "Hard” majors had higher mean scores for IH, Extra- version, Sensation, Extraverted Thinking, and Extraverted Sensation. "Soft" majors had higher mean scores for Feeling and Introverted Feeling. Although not highly significant, these results leave the impression that ”hard” majors tended to be Extraverted with strong Thinking and Sensation func- tions, whereas "soft” majors tended to be Introverted with a more highly developed Feeling function. The relationship between sex and major was also examined. Of the 37 ”hard" majors, 25 were male, whereas 45 36 Table 10. Mean Differences Between Hard and Soft College Majors for Hemisphericity, Attitude, Function, and Cognitive Mode Scores (df = 106) Hard Major Soft Major N = 37 N = 71 Hemisphericity/Attitude/ Function/Cognitive Mode Mean SD Mean SD t Integrated Hemisphericity 96.27 19.08 89.39 15.76 2.00** Extraversion 47.62 2.05 46.93 1.74 1.85* Thinking 25.97 2.21 25.35 1.77 1.59 Sensation 25.03 1.32 24.95 1.20 1.73* Introverted Thinking 13.71 1.37 13.56 1.23 .54 Extraverted Thinking 12.27 1.37 11.79 1.03 2.06** Extraverted Sensation 11.62 .92 11.14 .89 2.61*** Left Hemisphere 100.70 19.64 105.65 19.83 -1.23 Right Hemisphere 103.27 21.28 105.52 18.12 -.58 Introversion 52.59 1.96 53.04 1.62 -1.27 Feeling 24.57 1.85 25.23 1.55 -1.96* Intuition 24.65 1.64 24.80 1.94 -.40 Extraverted Feeling 11.92 1.19 12.14 1.07 -.98 Introverted Feeling 12.65 1.32 13.08 1.16 -1.77* Introverted Intuition 12.83 1.07 12.94 1.34 -.42 Extraverted Intuition 11.81 1.08 11.86 1.16 -.21 Introverted Sensation 13.41 1.01 13.45 .82 -.25 *p<.10 **p<.05 37 of the 71 "soft" majors were females. A 2 x 2 contingency analysis yielded a Chi-Square of 9.35, significant at p<.01. DISCUSSION Measures As discussed in the preceding section, significant weaknesses were identified with both the HIP and the SLIP measures central to this study. HIP's forced-choice nature yielded hemisphericity measures that averaged only 56 per- cent nonconfounded variance. While the SLIP was not forced- choice, it still manifested notable internal inconsistencies in some intercorrelations between its cognitive mode scores and also appeared importantly biased toward IntroversiOn. Despite these serious psychometric problems, several weak to modest linkages between hemisphericity and function were identified. The strongest relationships found in the present study were the bipolar relationships between the functions and these accounted for about 11 to 36 percent of the var- iance. The hemisphericity and Jungian typology linkages were weaker, at best accounting for 10 percent of the variance. The Functions and Right Hemisphericity The results partially supported the general hypothesis of linkages between hemisphericity and Jungian typology. This relationship appears to be strongest between RH and the Thinking and Intuition functions. The following results sup- ported this conclusion: a) the highly significant (p<.001) 38 39 positive correlation between Intuition and RH, and the highly significant (p<.001) negative correlation between Thinking and RH; b) the finding that Intuitives had higher RH scores than Thinkers; c) RH dominant individuals had higher Introverted Intuition scores than did LH-dominant individuals; d) Introverted Nonrationals had higher RH scores than did Extraverted Rationals. In addition to these hypothesized findings, RH correlated unexpectedly and pos- sibly spuriously with Feeling (positively) and Sensation (negatively). These results partially replicated Sibley's (1980) findings at Ball State University using the HIP Survey and the Meyers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). Assessing 90 under- graduate and graduate college students, Sibley reported that RH showed the following weak correlations: Thinking (r = -.15, 2E)! Feeling (r = .17, p<.05), Sensation (r = -.19, p<.05) and Intuition (r = .22, p<.05). Thus, two independent studies have identified a congruent pattern of weak linkages to RH. One possible explanation for the unexpected direction of the correlations between RH and the Sensation and Feeling functions is that the bipolar relationship between the two Rational functions (Thinking and Feeling) and between the two Nonrational functions (Intuition and Sensation) may have created a spurious relationship between RH and the Feeling and Sensation function. For example, the small negative cor- relation (-.24) between RH and Sensation may be the result 40 of the larger (-.44) inverse relationship between Sensation and the Intuition function, which is positively correlated (.32) with RH. If this were the case, then the correlation between RH and Sensation would be expected to approach 0 when Intuition is held constant. This is exactly what hap- pened. A similar outcome occurred when a partial-correlation was computed between RH and Feeling while controlling for Thinking. Similar spurious relationships likely played an even greater role in Sibley's study because the MBTI instru- ment that was used had implicit bipolar relationships between Thinking and Feeling scores and between MBTI Intu- ition and Sensation scores. Another possible explanation for the unexpected rela- tionships between RH and the Sensation and Feeling functions is that the hypotheses of the present study were incorrect. Sensation may actually be a Rational function and Feeling may be Nonrational. The most reasonable conclusion that can be drawn from Sibley's and the present findings is that both the Rational Thinking function and the Nonrational Intuition function are related to RH as predicted. But Jung's bipolarity assump- tion appears principally responsible for the relationship between RH and the Feeling and Sensation function. Further- more, the question of the Rationality or Nonrationality of the Feeling and Sensation functions and their relationship to hemisphericity require further study and clarification. The strongest present evidence supporting the concept of 41 opposite Rational and Nonrational functions were the signif- icant (p<.001) inverse relationships between Thinking and Intuition (r = -.60) and between Feeling and Sensation (r = -.33) (see Figure 1). The Functions and the Left Hemisphere Perhaps the most puzzling outcome was the lack of sig- nificant (p<.05) linkages between LH and any Of the func- tions or cognitive modes. Using the MBTI, Sibley (1980) reported the following series of correlations between LH and Thinking (r = .10), Feeling (r = -.10), Sensation (r = .46, p<.005) and Intuition (r = -.48, p<.005). The only present evidence supporting a relationship between Jungian typology and LH was the finding that LH dominant individuals had higher Introverted Thinking scores than RH—dominant individuals. Perhaps limitations of the SLIP and HIP contributed to the lack of a clearer relationship with LH. However, a cursory comparison (see Appendix C) of the individual items constituting the SLIP Thinking and the HIP LH measures that were expected to be related, revealed much content similar- ity in these items. Both Thinking and LH items included ref- erences to planning, organizing, and a step-by-step analysis in problem-solving details. Their major difference was more frequent references to verbal processing in the LH items. This marked content similarity between items raises the issue of to what extent the differences between the HIP Survey and the SLIP were conceptual versus semantic in 42 .mmHEOOOAOOHOH mo numeouum O>Humfimu mouoofloca mmmcxoflnu mafia .mcoflumflouuoo O>Hummmc 302m mmcfla :mxoun .wcofiumflmuuoo m>fluflmom 305m mocfifl Ofiaom .mmusmmmz Az .m.m .ev mHAm cam And a mm. >m>usm mnx coumwoomw< aaucm0wmacmfim mo mocmflum>oo .H ousoflm 43 nature. Are hemisphericity and Jungian typology actually separate but related systems or are they the same system being described with different words? Only further research can answer this question. Another possible explanation for the absence Of LH cor- relates may be sample bias. In recent years, much popular information has been released about hemisphericity and left/right brain functioning (Levy, 1985). It is conceivable that there may be a bias in the college student population used in this study toward appearing more intuitive and ”right-brained." The intuitive, right brain functions may be more in "vogue" now than the rational, thinking, "left- brained" image that has been pOpular in the past. This con- sideration is worth investigation. One approach would be to measure participant's self-image or attitude regarding cog- nitive style and see if it is related to their responses. If self-image or attitude was an extraneous variable it could be controlled using partial-correlations. Attitude and Hemisphericity The lack of significant linkages between Introver- sion/Extraversion and hemisphericity may be due to flaws in SLIP's measures of Introversion and Extraversion. As men- tioned earlier, when participants were classified on the basis of their SLIP Introversion and Extraversion scores, 108 were classified as Introverts whereas only 6 were Extra- verts and 3 had tie scores. Classification on the basis of dominant cognitive mode (e.g., an individual whose dominant 44 cognitive mode was Introverted Thinking would be classified as an Introvert) resulted in 97 Introverts but only 12 Extraverts with 8 ties. Thus, the SLIP appears biased towards Introversion. SLIP's authors (Singer & Loomis, 1984), explicitly cautioned users that the instrument is an experimental edition and will need further revision as more validational data accumulates. Therefore, while the present SLIP edition appears an advance over the MBTI in measuring the functions, additional improvements in the SLIP's measurement of Introversion and Extraversion appear needed. Consequently, the present study permits no conclusions regarding the relationship between attitude and hemispher- icity. Sibley's (1980) study did not include measures of Introversion and Extraversion. Sex Differences One of the most notable findings of the present study was the detection of strong individual differences in typol- ogy between males and females. The present males had signif- icantly higher mean scores for Extraversion, Thinking, Extraverted Thinking, and Extraverted Sensation, whereas females had higher Introversion and Feeling, and Introverted Feeling scores. Sibley (1980) also reported higher mean Thinking scores for males and higher mean Feeling scores for females. These findings offer interesting support for observa- tions made by Jung (1971) and others (Stevens, 1983) that Thinking and Sensation tend to be more dominant in men, 45 whereas Feeling (Introverted Feeling in particular) and Intuition tend to be more dominant in women. They also sup- port Wyckoff's (1974) contention that there are socialized differences in scripts between men and women. According to Wyckoff, men are programmed to conform to life scripts in which they are rational, thinking persons, while women are reinforced for being nonrational and feeling. This raises the question as to what degree these findings reflect real differences in social scripts between the sexes and to what extent these cultural biases may have been incorporated into the SLIP and MBTI. The fact that similar results were Obtained with two different instruments would tend to sup- port the concept of real differences in typology and life scripts between males and females. Further research is needed to clarify this issue. College Major The pattern of obtaining predicted outcomes for RH but not for LH was also relevant to the partial replication of Bakan's (1969) report. Like Bakan, the present study found that RH-dominant individuals more often selected ”soft” majors. Unlike Bakan, however, the present study did not find that LH-dominant individuals chose "hard" majors more Often than others. Methodological differences may partially account for this discrepancy. Bakan used direct observation of the direction of lateral eye movements as his hemisphericity indicator, but the present study used the self-report HIP 46 Survey. Studies comparing the HIP Survey with the Conjugate Lateral Eye Movements (CLEM) test have either failed to find significant linkages (Riegel, 1980) or have found only a partial relationship (Alberts & McCallum, 1982). Additional- ly, the present classification of ”soft” and "hard" majors was not identical to Bakan's. For example, the present study classified Business majors as "soft," while Bakan's sample had no Business majors. While some relationship between hem- isphericity and choice of college major appears likely, more research is needed to resolve these methodological issues. On the other hand, there is considerable documentation of the relationship between Jung's typology and career choice (Meyers, 1980). In fact, the battery of tests that career and vocational counselors give to clients often includes a measure of Jungian typology, indicating its popu- larity. The present finding that "hard" majors tended to be more Extraverted and prefer Thinking, while "soft" majors tended to be more Introverted and have higher Feeling scores, appears reasonable. But these results may also have been due to women being more attracted to ”soft” majors while males were more attracted to "hard" majors. A compar- ison of Tables 9 and 10 shows a similar pattern of signif- icant mean differences for females and ”soft" majors. Further clarification of this relationship and its meaning was beyond the sc0pe of the present study. Other Findings The detection of strong inverse relationships between Thinking and Feeling and between Sensation and Intuition, 47 when these functions were allowed to covary freely with one another, was evidence supporting the validity of Jung's bipolarity assumption. Past studies (Singer & Loomis, 1984; Mahlberg, 1982) have questioned the universality of the bipolarity issue by finding that individuals with a dominant Thinking function sometimes do not have Feeling as their inferior function and vice versa. The same was found for Intuition and Sensation. While it is clear that these func- tions are not always polar Opposites within the individual, the present study supports Jung's general contention that there are two pairs of bipolar functions, Thinking/ Feeling and Sensation/Intuition. Implications Hemisphericity and Jungian typology focus on the indi- viduals receiving and processing of information and thus relate to nearly all aspects of living. Both theories of human information processing or cognitive style have found important application. Hemisphericity has been applied in such diverse areas as problem solving (Doktor, 1978), learn- ing styles (Reynolds & Torrance, 1978), creativity (Tor- rance, 1982), and management training (Herman, 1981; Lynch, 1982). Reviews by Meyers (1980) and Carlyn (1977) show that Jung's typology has been used in educational, vocational and marriage counseling, in research on learning styles, in screening applicants for jobs, in selecting applicants for college and professional schools, and in predicting aca- demic success. Furthermore, hemispheric functioning has 48 been related to classical psychodynamic conflicts and processes such as dissociation and denial (Galin, 1974). And both hemisphericity and Jungian typology have been proposed, but not yet tested, for use in matching therapist and tech- nique to the client in psychotherapy (Quenk & Quenk, 1982; Rossi, 1977). The present work is an early step in exploring the linkage of these two notable theories. As this relationship becomes clearer, these two constructs could be used together to better match an individual's cognitive strengths and weaknesses with his or her present tasks and future goals. In addition, further clarification of the relationship between hemisphericity and Jungian typology could lead to the development of a more fully integrated and useful theory of human information processing. APPEND I CES APPENDIX A COMPARISON OF LEFT, RIGHT BRAIN DICHOTOMIES AND RATIONAL/NONRATIONAL FUNCTIONS 49 APPENDIX A A Comparison of Left, Right Brain Dichotomies (Bogen, 1969, Springer & Deutsch, 1981) and Dichotomies Describing Jung's Rational/Nonrational Functional Types (Jung, 1971) Jung's Jung's Rational Nonrational Left Brain Functions Right Brain Functions Intellectual Thinking Sensuous Sensation Intellect Thinking Intuition Intuition Rational Rational Perception Perception Objective Objective Existential Existential Directed Directed APPENDIX B SPEARMAN CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS FOR HIP SURVEY AND SLIP 5() no.vm .3“... t coauunucn cauuo>ouuxu no~.c u eouao-cow vauuo>uuuxu do.) ea... c uc‘.oou ocuuo>ouuxu oun.n nvm. .~.u a oeaxcush vouuo>ouuxu ova. own.u o_~.u son.) I :oquaauc— vauuo>ouueu o~n.u no. uo~.o u-.a ~O.u a couuo-eum vauuo>0uue~ mo.a nv~.o mo. nom.n ~o.n mo.| u usa~uou venuo>0uac~ can.u an. n¢~.l o—n. nov.I mo. no~.u o ocuxeuzh vauuo>0uu:_ one. no..- -.I uvv.u can. con.o ~o.u emm.n u couuaaueu an... amp. nc~.- .o. nm~.- awe. on..- ~_. as..- - :o.uaocom oc.- o~n.u «we. a.... v~.- o~.- ooh. cam.- o~.- emm.- . ecu-ooh so..- oo~. nm~.a can. no..- so.. on..- com. ooo.- o.. aom.- . o:.x:a:n now. own. adv. on. adv.l cmv.| nov.| o_.| —~.I mo.) hc.l Oo~. I :oquuo>auuuu no~.u awn. nu..- u~m.u Gav. omv. mov. cu. _~. mo. so. Oo~.I o.~u i :Ownu0>0uu:~ o..- o-. -.u on~. oc~.- so.- we.. no“. aa~.- -. ~_.u n.~ so. no.- u RSSUAuoaaahnoa ecu-coo»:— nnw. u-.a no. u-.r nvm. m_.n OO~. nm~.: o~.. nv~.u boa. eon.i mo.n No. ovv.u a >uauquocnuqloz anon: «o. oo. ~o.n so.l no.| dd. n~.I cu. mo.t co. n~.0 cc. vo.I vc. avm.l eav.l I auuuuuozanatoz yum; 2m mm mm Bu 2— m— m— h. uqaucn mcwm ~m~m xcuze m — =~ z: :4 qum use mm>usm AH: you mucoflofimwoou coflumflouuou cmeumomm m xHozmmm< APPENDIX C COMPARISON OF REPRESENTATIVE SAMPLE OF HIP LEFT HEMISPHERE (LH) ITEMS AND SLIP THINKING ITEMS 51 uofl>ocon >5 mmcmno H mm Mammxe cum3ou ou :mHQ mfiflmc m as new mcflnumcm >59 H muowmn m3OH> m.:omummwOHmm on» umpflmcou wmo ocfiuusm coma o>mn H ocfiNHcmmuo cam quaccmfim can no osom on wamom we Oum3ou :OHmmoumoum moumlwnlmoum m ocflfluoo cam mo>fluOOnno we amaumao mcoc on ou mpooc pon3 wauooxm cflmmem can uo>o oxmu ou ocooeom wcocm mOapaquum umm cam uomnoum uxm: woo swam Op mafia on» om: Amvv .mm. Amy Anav Amoav .mav mcflccmflm cam ocfiumcuo coooucu haaooflumsoummm Gamma ou wowoum ummn mxuoz zofln3 mcwumuumcoeoo Ono muOOOOuQ Hmuo>om mcwummeoo :ofiumEuOMOA man an cmocooamcfi :muwo umoe em .mucmemmfluuw>pm mewsmfl> cwnz mmfiufi>wuom HmOOmuom cam mem muwcmmuo 0» bag» om: xfifimwucosqom mmcwnu onwcmmuo ou oxflq wCOHUUDhumCH H MQHO> Hmwmhm haamoflumwfimmu spam on can Ono: Amm. Amav AmH. AHHV AON. Am. Aumnesz EmuH. wsouH ocwxcflna qum .umnesz souH. memuH mg mHm mewuH mcflxcflna mflflm cam mEOOH Ana. ouozmmfleom puma OH: HO onEmm w>wumucmmwumom mo OOmwnmmEOO U xHozmmmfl REFERENCES REFERENCES Alberts, F.L., Jr., & McCallum, R.S. (1982). Information exchange: The relationship among three measures of cognitive style. Clinical Neuropsychology, 3(2), 70-71. Arndt, S., & Berger, D.E. (1979). Cognitive mode and asymmetry in cerebral functioning. Cortex, lg, 78-86. Bakan, P. (1969). Hypnotizability, laterality of eye movements and functional brain asymmetry. Perceptual and Motor Skills, gg, 927-932. Bever, T.G. (1975). Cerebral asymmetries in humans are due to the differentiation of two incompatible processes: Holistic and analytic. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 263, 251-262. Bogen, J.E. (1969). The other side of the brain: II. An appositional mind. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society, 34, 135-162. Bogen, J.E., DeZure, R., Tehnouten, W.D., & Marsh, J.F. (1972). The other side of the brain: IV. The A/P ration. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society, g1, 49-61. Carlyn, M. (1977). An assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 5;, 461-473. Davis, L.T., & Johnson, P.J. (1983). An assessment of conscious content as related to introversion- extraversion. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 3(2), 149-168. Doktor, R.H. (1978). Problem solving styles of executives and management scientists. TIMS Studies in the Management Sciences, g, 123-124. Ehrlichman, H., & Weinberger, A. (1978). Lateral eye movements and hemispheric asymmetry: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, gs, 1080-1101. Fischer, R. (1975). Cartography of inner space. In R.H. Siegel & L.J. West (Eds.), Hallucinations: Behavior, Experience and Theory (pp. 197-239). New York: Wiley & Son. 52 53 Galin, D. (1974). Implications for psychiatry of left and right cerebral specialization. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 572-583. Galin, D., & Ornstein, R. (1972). Lateral specialization of cognitive mode. Psycholoysiology, 3 (4), 412-418. Ghosh, J.A. (1980). A comparison of cognitive styles of mathematically, musically, and artistically talented adolescents. Dissertation Abstracts International, 33, 5789-A (University Microfilms No. 80-10584). Herman, N. (1981). The creative brain. Training and Development Journal, 33 (10), 10-16. Jung, C.G. (1971). Psychological Types (R.F.C. Hull, Trans.). Collected Works (Vol. 6). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Original work published 1921.) Kazdin, A.E. (1980). Research design in clinical psychology. New York: Harper & Row. Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding. American Psychologist, 33, 411-420. Levy, J. (1985, May). Right brain, left brain: Fact and fiction. Psychology Today, pp. 38-44. Loomis, M. (1982). A new perspective for Jung's typology: The Singer-Loomis inventory of personality. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 31, 59-69. Loomis, M., & Singer, J. (1980). Testing the bipolarity assumption in Jung's typology. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 33, 1-6. Loomis, M., & Saltz, E. (1984). Cognitive styles as predictors of artistic styles. Journal of Personality, 33, 22-35. Lynch, D.J. (1982). Brain strategies: Applications for change and innovation. Training and Develgpment Journal, 33 (8), 62-68. Metzner, R. (1980). Correlations between Eysenck's, Jung's, and Sheldon's typologies. Psychological Reports, 31, 343-348. Metzner, R., Burney, C., & Mahlberg, A. (1981). Towards a reformulation of the typology of functions. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 33, 33-49. 54 Meyers, I.B. (1962). The Meyers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Meyers, I.B. (1980). Gifts differing. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Nebes, R.D. (1974). Dominance of the minor hemisphere in commissurotomized man for the perception of part-whole relationships. In D.M. Kinsbourne and W.L. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function (pp. 155-164). Springfield: C.C. Thomas. Ornstein, R.E. (1972). The psychology of consciousness. San Francisco: Freeman. Quenk, A.T. 8 Quenk, N.L. (1982). The use of psychological typology in analysis. In M. Stein (Ed.), Jungian analysis (157-172). LaSalle: Open Court. Riegel, T.R. (1980). Laterality of high school students and its relation to convergent and divergent abilities. Dissertation Abstracts International, 39, 5798-A (University Microfilms No. 80-10620). Reynolds, C.R. 8 Torrance, E.P. (1978). Perceived changes in styles of learning and thinking (hemisphericity) through direct and indirect training. Journal of Creative Behavior, 33, 247-252. Rossi, E. (1977). The cerebral hemispheres in analytical psychology. Journal of Analytical Psyghology, 33, 32-57. Schwartz, 8., 8 DeMattei, R. (1982, February). Mind: PSI-OI report. Omni, pp. 160-161. Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Silbey, V. (1980). Hemisphericityiand performance on the Meyers-Briggs. Unpublished manuscript, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana. Singer, J. 8 Loomis, M. (1984). The Singer-Loomis Inventorygof Personality. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Sperry, R.W. (1974). Lateral specialization in the surgically separated hemispheres. In F.O. Schmitt 8 F.G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences (pp. 5-19). Cambridge: MIT Press. Springer, S.P. 8 Deutsch, G. (1981). Left brain, right brain. San Francisco: Freeman. 55 Steiner, C.L. (1974). Scripts people live. New York: Grove Press. Stevens, A. (1983). Archetypes. New York: Quill. Taggart, W. 8 Torrance, E.P. (1984). Human Information Processing Survey. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. Torrance, E.P. (1982). Hemisphericity and creative functioning. Journal of Research and Develgpment in Education, 33 (3), 29-37. Torrance, E.P., Reynolds, C.R., Riegel, T., 8 Ball, 0. (1977). Your style of learning and thinking, forms A and B: Preliminary norms, abbreviated technical notes, scoring keys, and selected references. Gifted Childguarterly, 33, 563-573. Wheelwright, J.E., Wheelwright, J.H., 8 Beuhler, J.A. (1964). Jungian type survey: The Gray-Wheelwright test. San Francisco: Society of Jungian Analysts of Northern California. Wyckoff, H. (1974). Sex role scripting in men and women. In C.S. Steiner (Ed.), Scripts People Live (pp. 165-175). New York: Grove Press. Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 33, 151-175. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES lllWNWllllHI“WWII“W"WWW 31293007003902