.fi. ,,- “ -5“; sur—n— w 47’“ .éfé-n .7 . 4-44—1Y_.__ EHN)‘ «(I - ‘11”1‘ Y "‘" 3"!“ ‘11‘111111111i11‘f‘;1.?!‘311. “‘1 , )1! It'll?!" ,I \.: $‘1I‘I’;""Yf’fi“‘ ‘11,;1 . l 1‘ 2; 3'53 3:71;: 2 N 2“. ,ot‘h 1‘17‘1 cup—p -- jam "’5‘ . .1111..,41.11}.1u 1‘ 114111;, 14;: (~1- , 1:. ...€~ :1." t' "J" a 1.:I u ff. . 5.411.212 ' 1. v. ' v ‘ ‘1“ ’1, :.| .’ a“ I? “ 1" 4 . “1&1! 1:: '2 - D‘ . a "I . 1-3111. 9n 4, Mr- . .0- uvtr -.,._ :1. r - ‘5”. u4~ Y n :0” It" :4; ‘ “I amp. A' " i‘; ‘1 \L‘Lv’h“, ‘4‘? . \H‘ W!“ n"! ‘1 " ‘ ’4‘“ y 1 141mm}. 1,‘ L'l;}‘::;s! : l»: 4' .11'2'31'7' ‘ l-Ji '2?" 1 “$5 ' .. 3i {2‘37} . 31.3.; :2": 75:22.55 it: ’ u‘fr'. ' .3” :8? ~,' ‘ 3t {1 3.2.! i‘fillf' , . '1 'l (_ "1 1 , i "in: it” . 1&3“ 9-“ L‘ 4‘15 f LQ: x:a_ . ,QE . 4.2.4411 1 1 , .- .. -4. ‘: :gsic ' :gl‘8 ~ 1‘ I. 7 3:4 W ... 7 9’0" Pd‘“ " {fry 3“}: 1471;! .~. ‘91“, ,1"? ti} 2' 1:3,)? .4- 2“” . q-J‘l {.1"i¢,.(i 41,4 _, ,1 4i“ 7. (35’ £53.11": §;:( - v 15111111,? 2:332 “" 4.5%?" Erik-i; viii 3:51. ”.4; a: #93 ..C-c—vn ‘ .4 a’; 4 ‘A . 1t ‘1 $213; 3% 7i,“ 3!“ 1‘?! £95 9.1 .4 . $3 I. ' it 1 1 . _n 7 .;A_ ‘33. 7 6»: i‘_ 7n“: . .12 21:" I" ' :z. '1'! JI‘ 1?: us- ;- r 1 ' "2 2 ‘ 12. I i: :4" . 11 1, .15 -s 1. 'V 577'- IIUHHUIHIlllUllWHHWIHIIIHIIHHIIHNIINHIIWI 1293 00759 1658 Thisistocertifythatthe dissertation entitled AN INVESTIGATION OF THE INFLUENCE OF SELF-OTHER FEEDBACK CONGRUENCE 0N FEEDBACK ACCEPTANCE presented by Mary Beth DeGregorio has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph-D- degreeinhxnhalngL MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before due due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE U3 3’” by 4- FEE—fl TLIIJLJ MSU Is An Amman,- ~33quan Opportunity Institution chna-p.‘ AN INVESTIGATION OF THE INFLUENCE OF SELF-OTHER FEEDBACK CONGRUENCE ON FEEDBACK ACCEPTANCE By MaryBeth DeGregorio A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1990 I l A c. l bofiq ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF THE INFLUENCE OF SELF-OTHER FEEDBACK CONGRUENCE 0N FEEDBACK ACCEPTANCE By MaryBeth DeGregorio Performance feedback acceptance is generally considered an integral component in the performance feedback process. Little research has considered the factors that influence acceptance of feedback from an external source or the consequences of external feedback acceptance. The present research used a control systems perspective to investigate the i influence of congruence between external feedback and self-assessments of performance on feedback acceptance. College students were given false feedback regarding their performance on an in—basket exercise. This feedback was varied in terms of feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and source credibility. Results showed that (a) feedback sign was positively related to feedback acceptance, (b) feedback discrepancy was negatively related to feedback acceptance, (c) self-confidence interacted with feedback discrepancy to influence feedback acceptance, (d) external feedback interacted with acceptance of external feedback to influence consensus judgments made regarding performance and (e) self-assessment of performance carried more weight than external judgments of performance in influencing consensus performance judgments. Implications of these findings as well as future research needs and study limitations are discussed. Coryright by MaryBeth DeGregorio 1990 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Arriving at this point could never have been accomplished without the support and encouragement of many people. I would first like to thank my committee members John Hollenbeck, Dan llgen and Steve Kozlowski for their thoughtful commentary in reviewing my work. Their efforts helped to improve the quality of this product. I would especially like to express my deepest gratitude to my committee chairperson Kevin Ford. Kevin’s faith in my ability and desire for my success were ‘, motivating forces in the completion of this project. I am additionally grateful for the time and effort he put into this dissertation project. (Great turn around time Kevin!) I would also like to acknowledge the assistance of Sandra Lee, Sharon Sosnowski and Sandy Wanek in collecting and entering the data. You made the boring task of data collection tolerable and provided a sounding board for the complaints of a frustrated doctoral student. I hope this experience was educational and didn’t discourage you from pursuing advanced degrees. My sincere appreciation also goes to my friends in East Lansing, Chicago and elsewhere. The dissertation process can be a solitary one, iv however, I always knew I had friends to turn to when the going got tough. You saved my sanity with matinee movies, evening coffee and conversation, jaunts to Chicago and of course constant encouragement. (Jill, Howard, MJ, John, Mary - Thanks!) A special thanks goes to Ed Mongillo, the best friend and confidant anyone could hope to have. He was with me at every obstacle I faced, both personal and professional, to nudge me over each hurdle. The interest he expressed in my professional endeavors was both encouraging and motivating. His tremendous patience, love and support greatly contributed to the success of this project and to my personal growth and strength. It was a long time coming, but I finally made it to Chicago and 1 I couldn’t be happier. Finally, I would like to thank my family without whom this accomplishment would never have been achieved. My family provided a safe haven during the most trying times of my academic career. The unconditional love they have given so freely has provided me with comfort and security throughout my life. My family has taught me that life is a series of choices and that I can choose to be whatever I would like. I thank them for their support in any decision I made and any direction I chose to pursue. I am sure that at times they had more confidence in me than I did in myself. They have made a significant contribution to the achievement of both my personal and professional goals and to the completion of this project. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................................... LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................... CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION .............................................................. Problem Overview ..... _ - ..... Performance Feedback Performance Feedback Acceptance Feedback Acceptance Outcomes ..... Self-Evaluation Change- - - -- -- Behavior Change -- ....... Feedback Acceptance Determinants .......... - Feedback Sign ....................................... Source Credibility ............. “‘ “ 3‘ 0000 vv::=:. Psychological Closeness ................ ............................. Self-Processes ............................................................................. Summary-- Feedback Source congruence--- Congruence Def CIency - -- - - -- Cause of Congruence Deficiency Motivational Theories ................................................... Cognitive Theories ......................................................... Outcomes of Congruence Deficiencies ................................ Control Theory _----- --__- -vvvv ' -v—vvvv. Primary Components of Control Theory ....................................... Organizational Applications of Control Theory. Additional Control Theory Components ----- - -- Control Hierarchies - - Controlled Quantity Status---- CHAPTER 2: MODEL AND HYPOTHESES ........ vii OOOOOOOOOO xi xxi 15 I7 18 18 20 2] 23 26 29 29 32 35 35 38 Goals and Performance Feedback Feedback Discrepancy Feedback Acceptance Source Credibility Self-Certainty Consensus Performance Judgment CHAPTER 3: METHOD Pilot Study 1 - ................... ........... Subiects va Tack Procedure-- Results Pilot Study 11 Subjects Task Procedure Results Primary Study Subjects Task- Procedure Variables and Measures Feedback Sign Feedback Discrepancy Perceived Feedback Congruence Source Credibility Perceived Source Credibility Self-Rating External Rating Consensus Performance Judgment External Feedback Acceptance Self-Feedback Acceptance Self-Certainty Self-Confidence viii 46 47 48 49 50 50 52 53 53 53 54 54 55 57 57 57 57 59 59 59 63 63 66 66 66 67 67 68 68 68 69 69 69 70 7O CHAPTER 4: RESULTS- -- - -- - - 71 Manipulation Checks - -- ........ 73 Hypotheses - -- -- ----- -- 75 Hypothesis 1 -- - - 75 Hypothesis 2 ------ -- -- - ....................... 76 Hypothesis 3 - - ------ 79 Hypothesis 4 ........................................................................................ 82 Hypothesis 5 ........ - - 85 Hypothesis 6 ........................................................................................ 88 Hypothesis 7 ........................................................................................ 90 Additional Results - - - - -- ............. 93 CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION ...................................................................... 98 Determinants of Feedback Acceptance ................................................... 101 Influences on Consensus Performance Judgment .................................. 104 i Research Implications .................................................................................. 110 Limitations ...................................................................................................... 114 APPENDICES Appendix A - In-Basket Exercise ................................................................ 117 Appendix B - Measures Completed ........................................................... 129 Informed Consent Form ............................................................................ 129 Introduction to Task ................................................................................... 130 Performance Dimensions ........................................................................... 131 In-Basket Self-Rating Form ...................................................................... 132 Self-Feedback Acceptance--- - -- ------ 133 Self-Certainty - - ..... - -- 134 In-Basket Evaluator’s Rating- ..................................... 135 Attitude Survey--- .......................................... 136 Perceived Congruence ................................................................................ 142 Perceived Credibility-------- - - 143 External Feedback Acceptance ................................................................ 144 Self-Rating Time IL..-“ - ..................................................... 145 Debriefing.- - -- ...... . ................................................................... 146 Appendix C - Credibility Manipulations Appendix D - Results of factor analysis of self-certainty and self-feedback acceptance scales LIST OF REFERENCES 147 148 149 LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations for Manipulated Variables ..... - -- - ..... 6O Table 2: Descriptive Statistics, Intercorrelations and Reliabilities of Dependent Measures ............................................................ 61 Table 3: Descriptive Statistics, Intercorrelations and Reliabilities of Dependent Measures ----- - - 72 Table 4: Means and Standard Deviations for Manipulated Variables 74 Table 5: Means, Standard Deviations and Cell Sizes for External . Feedback Acceptance by Feedback Sign, Feedback Discrepancy and Source Credibility -- - - -- 77 Table 6: Regression of External Feedback Acceptance on Feedback Sign and Feedback Discrepancy. - - -- - 81 Table 7: Regression of External Feedback Acceptance on Feedback Sign, Source Credibility and Feedback Discrepancy- - - - 84 Table 8: Regression of External Feedback Acceptance on Feedback Sign, Feedback Discrepancy and Self-Confidence ...................................... 87 Table 9: Regression of Consensus Performance Judgment on Self- Rating and Self-Confidence - - - - ---------- - - 91 Table 10: Regression of Consensus Performance Judgment on External Rating and External Feedback Acceptance ............................... 94 Table 11: Regression of Consensus Performance Judgment on Self- and External Ratings of Performance ------ - - -------------- - 97 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: The TOTE Unit- - ............................................... 30 Figure 2: Control System Model of Motivation ......................................... 33 Figure 3: Hierarchy of Control for Lighting 3 Lamp -- -- 36 Figure 4: Powers’ Hierarchy of Control ...................................................... 37 Figure 5: Self-Other Feedback Congruence Model.....- -- ------ . 44 Figure 6: Experimental Design- - ----- - - - 64 Figure 7: Plot of the Relationship Between Feedback Sign and External Feedback Acceptance...-.-...- - - - 78 Figure 8: Plot of the Relationship Between Feedback Discrepancy and External Feedback Acceptance ............................................................... 80 Figure 9: Plot of the Relationship Between Feedback Sign, Feedback Discrepancy and External Feedback Acceptance ....................................... 83 Figure 10: Plots of the Relationship Between Feedback Sign, Feedback Discrepancy, Source Credibility and External Feedback and External Feedback Acceptance -- - ------------ 86 Figure 1]: Plot of the Interaction of Self-Confidence and Feedback Discrepancy on External Feedback Acceptance 89 Figure 12: Plot of the Relationship Between Self-Rating, Self- Confidence and Consensus Performance Judgment ................................... 92 Figure 13: Plot of the Interaction Between External Feedback and External Feedback Acceptance on Consensus Performance Judgment .............................................................................................................. 95 Figure 14: Modified Model of Self-Other Feedback Congruence .......... 107 xii CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION Problem Overview Performance feedback is well established as an important determinant of effective performance (e.g. see llgen, Fisher and Taylor, 1979 for a review). Investigations of performance feedback have demonstrated its importance in areas such as job design (Hackman & Oldham, 1976), organizational development and motivation (Locke, 1968). llgen et a1. (1979) contend that the influence of feedback on behavior is a complex process. They propose several cognitive variables that intervene between feedback and performance. An important variable which mediates the feedback-performance relationship is feedback acceptance. Feedback acceptance refers to the recipients’ evaluation of the accuracy of the feedback that is provided. llgen et a1. (1979) point out that feedback acceptance is influenced by characteristics of the feedback source, message and recipient. One issue that has not been addressed by feedback researchers is the influence of congruence of the message from different feedback sources. It is likely that incongruent feedback from different sources influences feedback 2 acceptance (perceived feedback accuracy). The majority of organizational research investigating the influence of performance feedback has been concerned with the joint influence of feedback and goals on performance. Becker (1978) investigated the influence of goals and feedback on energy conservation. He found that participants with a difficult goal who were given information regarding the number of kilowatt hours they used, reduced their consumption significantly more than participants with difficult goals only. Other studies of .the joint effect of goals and feedback have reached similar conclusions (Erez, 1977; Komaki, Barwick & Scott, 1978; Strang, Lawrence & Fowler, 3 1978). Upon reviewing the literature regarding the relationship between goals and feedback, Locke and his colleagues concluded that "neither knowledge of results nor goals alone are sufficient to improve performance" (Locke, Shaw, Saari & Latham, 1981, pg. 135). Investigations of the influence of performance feedback from a goal setting perspective have generally overlooked the influence of variables which mediate the relationship between feedback and performance. One reason this issue has been overlooked is that these studies have primarily been conducted with tasks for which the criterion is quantifiable (llgen & Moore, 1987). Therefore, the feedback could be easily compared to goals in determining the adequacy of performance. As such, perceptual 3 processes on the part of the feedback recipient become less relevant. However, for many jobs, especially those for which few objective measures of performance exist, interpersonal feedback is relied upon (Larson, 1984). Much research attests to the fact that performance feedback from others is an important determinant of job performance and job-related attitudes (See llgen et al., 1979 for a review). The cognitive evaluation of the feedback, specifically feedback acceptance, becomes an important consideration when interpersonal feedback is considered and when objective information is not available (Festinger, 1954). A main assumption of self-management theorists is that individuals .- evaluate their gv_vn_ performance with respect to standards and provide self-feedback (Manz, 1986). Investigations have found the self to be the most available, useful and trustworthy source of feedback (Greller & Herold, 1975; Hanser & Muchinsky, 1978; Herold, Liden, & Leatherwood, 1987). Self-assessments of performance have also been found to influence affective, cognitive and behavioral reactions to feedback from other sources (Brickman, 1972; Shrauger, 1975; Swann & Read, 1981). The feedback individuals perceive at any given time will likely be a composite of information selectively obtained from a variety of sources and weighted by its perceived utility (Taylor, Fisher & llgen, 1984). The interaction between feedback provided by oneself and feedback provided 4 by other interpersonal sources could have important implications for the acceptance of performance feedback. A lack of congruence between these feedback sources could influence the relationships between the feedback itself, characteristics of the feedback source, characteristics of the feedback recipient and feedback acceptance. The purpose of this study is to investigate the influence of self-other feedback congruence on feedback acceptance. The congruence between self-evaluations of performance and other evaluations of performance is expected to interact with source credibility and individual self-certainty to influence feedback acceptance and the ultimate judgment one makes regarding his/her performance. The following section discusses the previous work which has been conducted on feedback acceptance. Limitations to this line of research are noted. Next, literature relevant to self-other feedback congruence is reviewed. Control theory is presented to provide a theoretical perspective for investigating the influence of feedback source congruence on feedback acceptance. A model is outlined which delineates the expected relationships and the predicted hypotheses are specified. Finally, the method, results and conclusions drawn from studying these relationships are presented. 5 Performance Feedback Performance feedback is information regarding how well goals are being met (Ashford & Cummings, 1983). While performance feedback is a relatively simple concept, it can be thought of in different contexts. Performance feedback can be assessed in terms of its purpose, in terms of individual cognitive and behavioral reactions to the feedback or in a number of other ways. The provision of performance feedback serves two primary functions. First, it provides information regarding the adequacy of performance. Informational feedback directs behavior toward a desired state or goal (Locke, et. al., 1981; Locke, 1968). According to Ashford and Cummings (1983) the informational value of performance feedback will vary as a function of the uncertainty an individual experiences about issues surrounding goal attainment. The second function of performance feedback is motivational (llgen et al., 1979). From this viewpoint feedback can act as an incentive, reward or sanction. From the perspective of the organization the .u_lt_in_1_a_t£ purpose of providing performance feedback is to enhance performance. 6 The simple provision of performance feedback may M lead to desired performance improvements. llgen et a]. (1979) posit feedback acceptance as an important mediator of the performance feedback- performance relationship. The degree to which performance feedback has performance enhancing effects is fundamentally a function of its acceptance. The following section discusses feedback acceptance outcomes and determinants. Although goal acceptance has received considerable attention as an important moderator of the goal setting-performance relationship (Barley, 1985; Erez & Zidon, 1984) feedback acceptance has received relatively little research attention as an important variable in the performance feedback-performance relationship. The research that has addressed feedback acceptance is discussed next and areas in need of more research attention are noted. Performance Feedback Acceptance Feedback acceptance refers to the feedback recipient’s belief that the performance evaluation provided is an accurate portrayal of his/her accomplishments (llgen et al., 1979). Feedback acceptance is an important organizational objective in so far as it leads to goal attainment. Outcomes associated with feedback acceptance are discussed next. Once it has been established that feedback acceptance leads to important 7 organizational outcomes, determinants of feedback acceptance will be outlined. Feedback Acceptance Outcomes llgen et al. (1979) propose that feedback acceptance plays an integral role in the relationship between the provision of performance feedback and performance improvements. They also posit the desire to respond to the feedback and intention to respond to the feedback as two important mediators of the feedback acceptance-performance relationship. Organizational research has not investigated the cognitive outcomes associated with feedback acceptance (desire or intention to respond to the E feedback) nor the behavioral outcomes of feedback acceptance (performance improvement). Stone and Stone (1984) suggested that greater feedback acceptance should result in more changes in self- perceptions and behaviors but did not empirically test this proposition. Self-Evaluation Change. Social psychological research has demonstrated that evaluative feedback that is perceived as accurate influences many dimensions of self-perception (see Shrauger, 1975 for a review). For example, Harvey and Clapp (1965) asked subjects to describe themselves, how they expected others to rate them,'and how they hoped others would rate them. They found that subjects with positive expectancies raised their self-ratings more following feedback that was 8 more positive than expected. Subjects expecting negative evaluations lowered their self-ratings more following feedback which was more negative than expected. Following strongly discrepant feedback, there were no differential changes in self-assessments. While feedback acceptance was not assessed in this study, their findings suggest that feedback acceptance can lead to changes in self-evaluations. Self-assessment change may be desirable from an organizational perspective. If self-views were completely unchanging, people would never gain new knowledge about themselves. A primary function of performance feedback is to provide information. When performance . feedback is accepted and changes in self-assessments occur, it is likely that the performance information will influence subordinates’ future performance. Behavior Change. Research has also overlooked the relationship between feedback acceptance and behaviors or behavioral intentions. llgen et al. (1979) suggest that once feedback is accepted, the intention to respond based on the feedback should be strong. The inclusion of the behavioral intention in their model recognizes the strong evidence that performance improvements are contingent on specific, difficult goals (Locke, 1968; Locke et al., 1981). If the feedback which has been accepted relates to specific, difficult goals, and the intention to improve 9 performance is evident, then barring any situational constraints, performance improvements should ensue. O’Reilly and Anderson (1980) reported that employees’ perceptions about the accuracy of their performance appraisal evaluations directly moderated the relationship between the feedback received and subsequent performance. Important organizational outcomes that can result from performance feedback acceptance have just been delineated. Since feedback acceptance can lead to cognitive and behavioral reactions by feedback recipients, the factors which stimulate feedback acceptance must be considered. There has been very little research attention identifying performance feedback acceptance determinants. Feedback Acceptance Determinants Eeedback Sign. Much research on performance feedback has been concerned with feedback sign (e.g. DeNisi, Randolf & Blencoe, 1983; Stone & Stone, 1984). A well accepted finding is that positive feedback is more likely to be accepted and more accurately recalled than negative feedback (llgen et al., 1979). For example, Stone, Guetal and McIntosh (1984) found sign of the feedback to be an important influence on feedback acceptance. They investigated the sequence of positive and negative feedback as it influences feedback acceptance. Their findings supported the hypothesis 10 that performance feedback conveyed in a positive-negative versus a negative-positive sequence is more accepted. They suggest that the defensiveness provoked by the initial provision of negative feedback (Kay, Meyer and French, 1965) blocks the likelihood of feedback acceptance. llgen et a]. (1979) suggest that the consistency of the feedback sign influences feedback acceptance. Consistency refers to whether the feedback presented in a performance evaluation session is all positive or negative (consistent) or a combination of the two (inconsistent). llgen et al. (1979) contend that when feedback is consistent, internal attributions are made and individuals are more likely to accept the feedback provided. A This proposal, however, has not been empirically tested. Source Credibilig. Ilgen et al. (1979) also identified source credibility as an important determinant of feedback acceptance. Credibility can be broken down into a variety of dimensions but an important one for feedback acceptance appears to be expertise of the source (Giffin, 1967). Investigations of rater expertise which are described below, reveal relatively consistent findings. Bannister (1986) carried out a study assessing the influence of performance outcome feedback on responses to feedback. One variable of interest in this study was credibility of the feedback source. Credibility was manipulated by telling undergraduate subjects that they were either 11 being evaluated by a 43 year old personnel vice president or by a college sophomore intern. Results indicated a small but significant difference between high and low source credibility on feedback acceptance, such that high credIbility led to higher feedback acceptance ratings. Stone et al. (1984) investigated the influence of feedback sequence and rater expertise on feedback acceptance. Source expertise was manipulated by varying the name, title and amount of experience possessed by the rater. Their findings revealed that when subjects believed ratings were provided by a high expertise source, greater feedback acceptance resulted than when subjects believed their rater had i a low level of expertise. Egchologjca] Closeness. Research indicates that not only the credibility of the source but the psychological closeness of the feedback source influences reactions to performance feedback. (Greller, 1980; Greller & Herold, 1975; Hanser & Muchinsky, 1978; Herold, et al. 1987). Performance feedback can be provided from a variety of potential sources ranging from the company, supervisor, co-workers, task and self (Hanser & Muchinsky, 1978; Herold & Greller, 1977). The formal provision of supervisory feedback is the most prevalent approach for disseminating performance related information that is used in organizations (Cedarblom, 1982). This type of feedback is relied upon for many jobs, however other 12 feedback sources may be perceived as useful. Studies have investigated the frequency or amount, consistency and perceived usefulness of feedback generated by a given source. Greller and Herold (1975) found that individuals relied most upon feedback sources that were psychologically close to them. According to Greller and Herold (1975), psychological closeness refers to the intrinsic nature of the feedback. Herold and Greller (1977) suggest that feedback sources can be arranged hierarchically according to psychological closeness from self, task, and co-worker to supervisor and organization respectively. Their findings suggest that individuals viewed feedback sources which were psychologically close as providing feedback that was more frequent, more consistent and more useful than psychologically distant sources of feedback. In a related study, Herold et a1. (1987) investigated how feedback recipients rate feedback sources on frequency or amount, consistency and perceived usefulness of feedback information. They found, consistent with previous research, that individuals viewed feedback from themselves (self- generated feedback) as more frequent, more consistent and more useful than any other source of feedback. Feedback from the task was next, followed by feedback from supervisors, co-workers and the organization respectively. As Herold et. al. (1987) note, little attention is paid to how 13 individuals can be their own sources of feedback because most organizational feedback systems are based on organizational and supervisory feedback. In addition, no research has yet looked at the influence of source of feedback on feedback acceptance. Barley (1988) conducted one of the few studies assessing performance as a function of feedback source. Subjects in his study were telephone salespeople working for a magazine publisher. In this investigation he manipulated feedback source (self, supervisor) and feedback specificity (told exceeded or underproduced goal level, told exact number sold). The feedback source which Barley identifies as self- ; generated feedback would be better termed task feedback. It was computer generated feedback sought by salespeople which reports how many subscriptions they have sold. Barley (1988) found that feedback from the computer led to superior performance compared to feedback provided from a supervisor. This finding provides preliminary evidence that feedback from psychologically closer sources may lead to greater performance improvements than feedback from more distant sources. The research on determinants of feedback acceptance leads to the following conclusions. Feedback acceptance is enhanced when the feedback provided is positive, when the feedback source is credible and when the source is psychologically close. j These research findings leave I 14 several issues unresolved. First, as Taylor, Fisher and llgen (1984) point out, organizations are most concerned with situations in which employees are performing below standard. Therefore, they should be most concerned with acceptance of negative feedback. Organizational research has not addressed how the acceptance of negative feedback can be enhanced. In addition, research has not addressed the possibility that feedback recipients may receive feedback regarding the same behavioral event from two or more sources. If this occurs, the potential for conflict exists. The majority of organizational feedback systems are based on i supervisory feedback, yet individual employees can and do assess their own performance. They also provide themselves with feedback regarding their performance which could potentially conflict with other feedback sources. The research by Herold, Greller and their associates indicates that individuals rely on self-feedback more than supervisory feedback. These investigations have found the self-feedback to be evaluated more positively than other sources of feedback (Greller & Herold, 1975; Hanser & Muchinsky, 1978; Herold, et al., 1987). Finally, the interaction of source credibility with other variables has received little attention. llgen et al. (1979) suggest that negative feedback may be more acceptable when the feedback source is credible but research has yet to be 15 conducted on this issue. Self-Processes. Research conducted in social psychology may help us to address issues left unresolved by organizational research. Research in social psychology suggests that individuals are most willing to accept feedback which is similar to their own self-view. According to self- consistency theory (Secord & Backman, 1961) this tendency results from an individual’s desire to maintain a consistent view of themselves. In fact, Swann and his colleagues (Swarm & Read, 1981, 1982) have found that individuals bias their search of self- relevant information toward the acquisition of confirmatory data. In ' addition, they found that the retention of self-confirmatory information was longer than for disconfirmatory information. Finally, confirmatory information was viewed as more accurate (i.e. more acceptable) than disconfirmatory information. Social psychologists have also identified self-certainty (Jones & Schneider, 1967; Marek & Mettee, 1972) as a potentially important determinant of feedback acceptance. Self-certainty refers to how confident an individual is in his/her self-assessment of performance. Jones and Schneider (1967) proposed that uncertainty regarding a particular ability results from unstructured cognitions about the ability. Therefore, individuals with low levels of self-certainty will be more receptive to 16 information (feedback) about their own competencies. Jones and Schneider (1967) induced subjects to adopt low self- appraisals of performance on a social sensitivity task. Certainty was manipulated by either providing subjects with detailed negative feedback (high certainty condition) or limited negative feedback (low certainty condition). They found that desire to receive self-confirmatory information was higher for those in the high certainty group than for those in the low certainty group. Implications from this study are limited as the manipulation of certainty used in this investigation is confounded with specificity of feedback, so it is unclear whether subjects preferred I. feedback because it was specific or because they were more self-certain. Maracek and Mettee (1972) also investigated the influence of self- certainty on reactions to feedback. They conducted a study in which subjects were asked to complete self-esteem questionnaires and to indicate their level of certainty about their responses. Individuals were then provided with success feedback. Subjects with low self-esteem who were certain about their self-assessment rejected or minimized success feedback and avoided future success while subjects who were uncertain accepted the success feedback. This finding suggests that self-certainty may differentially influence individuals’ receptivity to feedback which is incongruent with their self-views. 17 Summary. A consideration of the research findings in the organizational sciences and social psychology leads to several conclusions. First, characteristics of the feedback message, especially the influence of feedback sign, are well established as influencing feedback acceptance, but the acceptance of negative feedback has received little attention. The influence of both feedback recipient and source characteristics on feedback acceptance has received scant empirical investigation. In addition, the abundant evidence regarding superior and subordinate disagreements in performance assessment (See Fisher & Russ, 1987 for a review) suggests a need for studying the joint influence of source and recipient characteristics on feedback acceptance. Social psychology research suggests that disagreement between self and other sources may be a function of preference for self-confirmatory information (Swann, 1982). This preference is influenced by the certainty with which an individual holds his or her self-view (Marek & Mettee, 1972). Research has shown that feedback which is discrepant from one’s own self-view is less likely-to be accepted than feedback which is congruent with one’s self-view (Crary, 1966; Shrauger, 1975). A lack of consensus between self and other feedback sources should significantly influence the positive relationship traditionally found between feedback and performance. An understanding of the causes of feedback source 18 incongruence is needed to make predictions regarding the joint influence of the feedback recipient’s self-feedback and feedback provided by another. Feedback Source Congruence This section considers the issue of self-other congruence in assessing and evaluating performance. First, the need for an inquiry into self-other feedback congruence is described. Second, several theories that may provide explanations for a lack of congruence are noted. Finally, the influence of self-other feedback congruence on outcomes such as feedback acceptance is discussed. Congr_uence Deficiency Information regarding the adequacy of one’s own performance can be acquired in a variety of ways. The most common assumption is that performance information derives from the reaction of others to one’s own behavior. A critical component of an individual’s self-definition is the response of others to an individual’s actions. This is the premise upon which most organizational feedback systems are based. This presumption is also the concept from which symbolic interactionism (Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934) derives. Symbolic interactionists contend that individuals’ self-perceptions develop from how others view them (the looking glass self). 19 While self-definition may derive from the responses of others, this does not necessarily mean that the self and any other single individual will agree. In organizational settings, agreement between superior and subordinate perceptions of subordinate performance is critical (Taylor, Fisher & llgen, 1984). Research indicates that these individuals often do not agree. For example, llgen, Peterson, Martin & Boeschen (1981) assessed the correspondence between supervisors and subordinates reactions to performance feedback sessions. They found that supervisor and subordinate perceptions of these feedback sessions show lit_tl_e agreement. Supervisors’ evaluations of the session were much more favorable than those of subordinates. In addition, their results demonstrated that overall written descriptions completed by superiors and subordinates immediately after the feedback session of how the appraisal session had progressed were significantly different. Other evidence of self-other rating discrepancies comes from the self- appraisal literature. Levine, Flory and Ash (1977) found that self- appraisals have less variability than supervisor-based assessments. Numerous studies have also found that self-ratings tend to have higher mean values (leniency) than other appraisal methods (Prien & Liske, 1962; Thornton, 1968, 1980). In addition, self-assessments appear to 20 exhibit less halo error (defined as the intercorrelation among dimensions) than superior ratings of performance (Lawler, 1967; Parker, Taylor, Barrett & Martens, 1959). Harris and Schaubroeck (1988) recently conducted a meta-analysis of the relationship between self- and superior ratings of performance. Their results revealed only a moderate (.35) correlation between self- and superior assessments of performance. In summary, these studies demonstrate that self- and superior ratings of performance are only somewhat related (Harris & Schaubroeck, 1988). If self-assessments are not solely based on the viewpoints of others, it would be useful to explore other bases for making self-evaluations. b Causes for Congr_uence Deficieng Several theories have been proposed as explanations for the low congruence between self- and other assessments of performance (See Fisher and Russ, 1986 for a review of superior-subordinate agreement). The following section briefly describes several theories of self-assessment. The purpose of this discussion is not to identify which theory provides the "true" explanation for a lack of congruence between self and other feedback sources. Instead it lays the groundwork for understanding the joint influence of self- and other feedback on outcomes such as feedback acceptance. 21 The basic premise underlying the theories of self-assessment is that individuals utilize either self-protective strategies (Strube & Roemmele, 1985) or make self-serving attributions for their performance (Bradley, 1978). The cause of this egocentric bias have been ascribed to either motivational or cognitive processes. In either case, this bias influences the nature of self-evaluations which are made. Motivational Theories. Selfoconsistency theorists contend that individuals attempt to maintain a fixed self-view (Korman, 1970). According to Korman (1970) individuals’ self-assessments of performance are based on their level of task related selfoesteem. If they have high task related self-esteem people will be motivated to seek feedback which indicates success. If individuals have low task related self-esteem they will be motivated to seek feedback which indicates failure, thereby confirming their initial self-view. Dipboye (1977) questions this strong consistency point of view. He suggests that research purported to support the self-consistency view can also support a self-enhancement view of work motivation. A crucial test of self-consistency predictions would involve persons who would seek consistency even at the cost of failure or other negative consequences. Some research evidence supports the view that individuals desire feedback which confirms initial self-expectancies (Shrauger, 1975). Swann 22 and Read (1981) found that people are more likely to attend to, encode and retrieve information if it is self-verifying. In addition, Swarm and Read (1981) gave subjects an opportunity to seek feedback from interaction partners that would either confirm or disconfirm their self- conceptions. Subjects displayed a clear preference for feedback that would confirm their self-perceived emotionality and assertiveness. Traditional conceptualizationss of self-enhancement theory (Smith, 1968) have suggested that people have a basic desire to think of themselves favorably. Self-consistency theory is a similar theoretical position which posits that people are motivated to sustain their self-conceptions (Swarm, ' 1982). The difference between these two positions can best be demonstrated in a negative feedback situation. Consistency theory states that negative feedback is accepted if an individual has negative expectancies (confirms expectancy of failure). Self-enhancement theory contends that negative feedback is not readily accepted because individuals are motivated to think of themselves favorably. Research also demonstrates that individuals may avoid evaluative information about ability if this information is damaging to one’s self- esteem. Subjects who believed that they were high in task related ability chose to engage in highly diagnostic tasks, but people who believed that they were low in ability avoided the opportunity to gather diagnostic 23 information (Meyer & Starke, 1982). In another study, subjects who were led to doubt their ability to maintain high scores similarly avoided diagnostic test items (Sachs, 1982). Cognitive Theog’es. Another theory that may help to explain the differences between self- and other ratings of performance is attribution theory. Self-attrrbutional bias is the tendency to attribute positive outcomes to oneself and negative outcomes externally to aspects of the situation beyond the individual’s control. Jones and Nisbett (1971) provide evidence that "actors" and "observers" differ in their perceptions of the causes of behavior, therefore their evaluations of behavior will differ. They assert that "there is a pervasive tendency for actors to attribute their actions to situational requirements, whereas observers tend to attribute the same actions to personal dispositions" (Jones & Nisbett, 1971 p. 2). Actors are likely to emphasize situational constraints, conflicting pressures and role limitations while observers are more inclined to personalize the cause for success or failure. Miller (1976) tested this proposition. Subjects were asked to take a social perceptiveness test. After testing was completed, subjects were either told that the test was a valid well established test or that it was a new, experimental and unvalidated examination. The experimenter then provided subjects with false feedback indicating that they had either done 24 well or poorly on the test. The results of the study indicated that successful outcomes were attn'buted more to internal factors and unsuccessful outcomes more to external factors. In addition, these tendencies were greater under high than under low ego involvement conditions. That is, high-involvement failure subjects (when test was presumably valid) made greater attributions for their performance to luck and less to ability and effort than did low-involvement failure subjects (when the test was presumably unvalidated). Similarly, high-involvement success subjects made greater attributions for their performance to ability than did low-involvement success subjects. Finally, investigations of the role of self-schemas in self-assessments also reveal a rationale for differences found between self-assessment and external assessments of behavior. Markus (1977) takes a cognitive perspective postulating that an individual utilizes "self-schemata" (cognitive generalizations of information about the self) to organize personal data. These schemata are thought to bias the type of information attended to and encoded into memory. In her study, some subjects were chosen who had well-developed schemas of themselves as being dependent or independent (schematic) and others who had not articulated self-schema on either of these dimensions. Those possessing schemas for independence processed schema relevant information (independence 25 related adjectives) more quickly than information that was unrelated to the schema (dependence related adjectives). Subjects with schemas for dependence showed exactly the opposite effect. Aschematics did not differ in their processing of either type of information. Rogers, Kuiper and Kirker (1977) also investigated the existence of self-schemas and the capacity of self-schemas to influence the encoding process. He asked some subjects to determine whether or not a series of sentences were self-descriptive and to use that decision as an aid to memory, while other subjects were not given those instructions. Subjects with these instructions had greater recollection of the sentences than those without the instructions. In a related study, Kuiper and Rogers (1979) investigated the processing of self- and other-referential information. Subjects in their study were presented with adjectives and asked to rate whether or not they were self-descriptive or descriptive of another. Their study demonstrated the self-referential adjectives were more quickly and easily recalled than other-referential adjectives. They concluded that self-referential data may be more cognitively available than other-referential information. The previous review of these theories suggests that the reason self- other feedback may differ can be explained in motivational or cognitive terms. First, individuals may wish to protect or enhance their own self- 26 esteem. Therefore, their self-evaluations would tend to be higher than those provided by other sources. This conclusion is supported by research which has found self-assessments to be more lenient that other assessments. A second explanation for self-other feedback differences may be due to information processing differences. Self-relevant information has been found to be easier to encode and recall than other information. There are clearly many explanations for the lack of congruence between self- and other assessments. Given this information we now turn our attention to outcomes associated with the lack of congruence between self and other feedback sources. Outcomes of Congruence Deficiencies Research supports the view that people prefer events that are predictable and consistent with their expectations (Snyder & Swarm, 1978). Of greatest interest here are data suggesting that the preference for information which confirms individuals’ view of themselves, influences reactions to self-relevant feedback from others. Shrauger (1975) reviewed numerous studies which showed that individuals accept information which confirms their self-views much more than disconfirmatory information. External evaluations consistent with an individuals’ self-view were also more accurately retained and given more credence than inconsistent feedback. It is interesting to note however, that affective reactions to 27 feedback did not necessarily indicate a preference for confirmatory feedback. Individuals showed higher levels of satisfaction with positive feedback than negative feedback even if initial self-evaluations were low. Feedback which confirms individuals’ self-perceptions is not only viewed as more accurate when received, but is actually sought after. Swarm and Read (1981) conducted a study in which subjects assessed their own assertiveness and emotionality. They hypothesized that self- assertive individuals would solicit assertive feedback and self-unassertives would solicit unassertive feedback. Similarly, those who viewed themselves as emotional would seek consistent feedback, while those who believed they were unemotional would seek self-unemotional feedback. Consistent with these predictions subjects were more likely to seek social feedback which confirmed their own view of themselves than information which disconfirmed their view. Follow-up investigations (Swarm & Read, 1981) revealed that individuals were even willing to pay for self- confirmatory feedback and regarded this type of information as especially informative. Taken together, the research which has investigated individuals’ responses to feedback which is incongruent with self-views suggests that this type of feedback may be rejected. People expect to receive feedback which confirms their self-expectancies. In fact, individuals seek 28 confirmatory feedback and may go to great lengths, such as paying for such information, in order to receive it. Swarm and Hill (1982) found that if feedback confirmed self-conceptions passive acceptance of the feedback resulted. However, if the feedback disconfirmed self- conceptions, they fervently resisted the feedback by demonstrating they were not as the feedback made them out to be. The fact that individuals tend to reject feedback that does not coincide with the feedback they provide for themselves has important implications for organizational feedback systems. This implies that subordinates will reject performance feedback that is inconsistent with how they believe they have performed. From an organizational perspective it may be desirable for subordinates to accept and act upon the feedback which is received from superiors. The next section presents the control theory model of self-regulation. Control theory is presented to provide an integrative framework for viewing message congruence among feedback sources. First, the basic tenets of control theory are presented. Then, organizational applications of control theory are discussed. Finally, control theory concepts are used to develop a model of individual reactions to self-other feedback congruence. 29 Control Theog Cybernetics (Wiener, 1948) or control theory is a general approach to the understanding of self-regulating systems. It is based on the premise that regulation occurs between a standard and external feedback. The application of control theory to psychological issues has been slow in coming (Miller, Galanter & Pribaum, 1960; Powers, 1973). Only recently has control theory been utilized in the organizational literature (Campion & Lord, 1982; Hollenbeck & Brief, 1987; Klein, 1988; Lord & Hanges, 1987; Taylor, et al., 1984) to provide an explanation for the motivating influence of goals and performance feedback. " Primag Comp_onents of Control fljheog The negative feedback loop is the fundamental element of control theory. Miller et al. (1960) present the TOTE unit to describe the functioning of the negative feedback loop (See Figure 1). TOTE is an acronym for Test-Operate-Test-Exit. This model suggests that determining the appropriateness of an existing state is achieved by first t_esti_ng for this condition. Testing is accomplished by comparing the current state with a referent state or goal. If a discrepancy exists then some ogration is required to bring the current state in line with the referent. Testing is repeated until the test is passed. When the test is passed, control is no longer needed so control exits from this system. 30 Figure 1. The TOTE Unit. Adopted from Miller, Galanter and Pribaum (1960). v- Q» 4 TEST Congruity \t Incongruity OPERATE 31 The feedback loop described above is said to be a negative feedback loop because the response to an error is an attempt to reduce the discrepancy (Powers, 1973). A positive feedback loop is a response to an error that attempts to increase the perceived discrepancy. Taylor et al. (1984) suggest that investigation of self-regulating systems in the workplace focus on discrepancy reducing systems. They point out that when considering the attainment of a performance goal, being above standard is desirable, however being below standard is undesirable. The typical illustration utilized to portray the logic of control theory uses a thermostat. A thermostat is a device which controls the ._ temperature in a given environment. The thermostat is set by its operator at a specific temperature such as 68 degrees. This temperature serves as the referent. 'Ihe thermostat then senses the environment to determine if the temperature is consistent with the previously set standard. If a discrepancy is detected between the referent or standard and the sensed temperature, the thermostat operates by turning on the heat When a discrepancy is eliminated, control of the system is not relinquished as could be deduced from the examination of a single TOTE sequence. The environment is continually monitored or "sensed" to determine if conditions have changed resulting in additional discrepancies. 32 Carver and Scheier (1981) term these changes to the existing environment "disturbances". For instance in the thermostat example, assume the thermostat operated and brought the room temperature in line with the previously set standard of 68degrees. Then someone opens a window which cools the room off considerably. This disturbance reactivates the control system to reduce the discrepancy between the sensed environment and the referent. Organizational Applications of Control Theog Organizational research utilizing control theory as a framework has been primarily concerned with control theory applications to motivation. Specifically, control theory has been presented as an explanation for the motivational properties of the interaction of goals and feedback (Campion & Lord, 1982; Lord & Hanges, 1987). Campion and Lord (1982) suggest that people regulate their own behavior by monitoring the environment (See Figure 2). Individuals compare referents or goals with sensed environmental (task) feedback. Any discrepancy perceived between the sensor and referent leads to self-correction. They identify two potential responses to goal-feedback discrepancies. A behavioral response would most likely result in increased effort or potentially a change in the strategy used to attain the goal. A cognitive response would be lowering the goal. The recognition that discrepancies 33 Figure 2. Control systems model of motivation (adopted from Campion and Lord, 1982). Referent (Goal) Referent Cognitive Signal Change _ error? "———"'__{Comparator Decision 1 - Mechanism Sensor Signal Behavior Change ,__J__. sensor l PERSON geffector — — — ENVIRONMENT Feedback Output (e.g. action) .L--————_ environment 34 can be reduced by altering the goal suggests that goal commitment is a necessary precursor to the performance enhancing effects of the interaction of goals and feedback. If goal commitment is present, a cognitive change or change of goal is unlikely. Campion and Lord (1982) investigated a control theory model of the relationship between goals and feedback. In their study they asked students to set goals for performance on five tests throughout the course of a term. Their findings indicated that the comparator was a key component in triggering changes in behavior or cognitions. Magnitude and number of failures were associated with behavioral changes such as increased effort and number and consistency of failures were associated with cognitive changes such as goal reduction. Their investigation incorporated control theory in it’s simplest form. The primary goal of the study was to test the viability of a negative feedback loop for explaining the relationship between goals and feedback. However, some of their findings cannot be explained by this elementary perspective. Specifically, a number of students raised their goals after initial failure suggesting that lower level goals may fulfill strategic functions in attaining more important higher level goals. This finding suggests the existence of a hierarchy of control. The notions of goal hierarchies, multiple goals and controlled quantity status are crucial to the 35 understanding of control theory. A discussion of these additional components to control theory is next. Additional Control Theog Components Qntrol Hierarchies. Complex control sequences can be described by hierarchies of feedback loops. Control theory research should attend to the hierarchical level at which behavior is operating. The combination of feedback loops can be illustrated by an example which uses the terminology set forth by Powers (1973) (See Figure 3). Consider attempting to turn on a lamp which will not light. The standard, or desired state, is for the light to be on. In determining how to "operate", to turn the light on, one may go through a series of tests. The first step in the sequence may be to test the light bulb in a different lamp. If the bulb works, one proceeds to another possibility. The second step could be to test the electrical outlet to determine if it is working properly. This could be done by testing a lamp in this outlet that you are certain works. If the working lamp lights then one moves on to the next possibility and so forth. It is apparent from this example that simple decisions can be linked in an lengthy chain of potential operations. The interconnection of feedback loops also suggest that a single behavior may aid in achieving a variety of referents. Powers (1973) 36. Figure 3. Hierarchy of control for lighting a lamp. Adapted from Powers (1973). [Is lamp on? no Check light bulb? F—ffiorks in another] Get new NO . - - lightbulb '5' B "U c E I Check outlet J L Another lamp works in this outlet? Hi} LGet new lamp 37 Figure 4. Power’s hierarchy of control. Adapted from Powers (1973). GOAL Becoming Educated Reading a Book Turning the Light Switch Hand on , Switch Turn of Switch Hand Grasps Switch Grasping Muscle Tensions Systems - Organized entities; models; beings. Principles Generalizations drawn from many different examples of lower-order perceptions; facts, heuristics, laws, beliefs. Programs - A network of choice-points characterized by tests at the nodes. Relationships - A regularity in the simultaneous space-time behavior of two or more independent lower-order elements. Sequence or - A fixed succession of lower-order Event elements; ordering. Transition - Time and space changes; partial derivatives. Configuration- An object, pattern, arrangement, or invariant of the present moment. Sensation - Quality of intensity; vector. Intensity - Magnitude of stimulation of sensory receptor; energy flow. 38 hierarchy provides the standard for the next lower level in the hierarchy. That is, turning on the lamp could be a standard in the middle range of a hierarchy of standards. A subordinate standard at the lowest level would be the muscle tension required to turn the switch. At higher levels, turning the lamp on could be part of a plan of operation to achieve a superordinate standard such as reading a book. Similarly, reading a book could be part of an operation designed to achieve an even higher order goal of becoming educated. At each level in the hierarchy, the outcomes of behavior are assessed by monitoring input information and comparing it with the referent which derives from the ‘ level above. At the lowest level the sensors indicate the present level of muscle tension and the comparator assesses whether the desired muscle tensions (referent) are being created. Similar processes occur at successively higher levels. Controlled Quantity Status. The notion of controlled quantity status concerns the issue of what discrepancy behavior is attempting to reduce. That is, attention should be paid to the level in the hierarchy of control at which behavior is operating. Behavior geared toward lighting a room will have no influence on the room temperature. The controlled quantity status (that which is being controlled) of the actor must be identified to explain behavior in control theory terms. Campion and Lord (1982) 39 assumed that the controlled quantity for subjects in their study was individual test score not overall course grade. This assumption led to the unexpected finding that subjects who failed to achieve individual test goals actually raised their subsequent goals. It may be that the controlled quantity for these individuals was overall course grade. While Campion and Lord (1982) were primarily concerned with the initial application of control theory to organizational issues, Hollenbeck and Williams (1987) addressed the issue of control quantity status in their investigation of control theory. They identified six facets of work which could serve as controlled quantities for department store salespeople. These facets were: 1) base pay, 2) job security, 3) the nature of the work itself, 4) co-worker relations, 5) job performance and 6) supervisory practices. Their findings indicate that the influence of goal level on performance was significantly higher for individuals with high performance control. That is, when job performance was the controlled quantity, the relationship between goal and performance was strengthened. These results support the view that not all perceptions require behavioral control, therefore it is necessary to consider the performance control variable which is operating. This finding suggests the possibility of expanding the control theory perspective to organizational issues outside of motivation. The research 40 by Hollenbeck and Williams (1987) suggests that standards other than performance can be controlled quantities. For example, individuals receiving performance feedback may wish to control the discrepancy between self-generated feedback and feedback received from external sources. Taylor et al. (1984) discuss the effects of feedback on individual reactions from a control theory perspective. They consider discrepancies between personal and organizational goals, the causes for these differences and potential ways of reducing these differences. They contend that performance feedback must closely correspond with the goal for performance feedback to result in desired performance improvements. In addition, the feedback recipient and feedback provider must agree on the goal or desired behavior. Taylor et al. (1984) posit that any incongruity between the goal of the feedback recipient and the goal of the feedback provider could result in feedback which is perceived by the recipient as inaccurate. However, an issue left unaddressed is the existence of self-other feedback congruence and the influence of congruence on feedback acceptance issues. Note that the model and proposed hypotheses presented next, do not attempt to provide a test of control theory. Instead, control theory is utilized as a framework for examining the performance feedback process. 41 The fundamental position of control theorists is that individuals regulate their behavior in relation to standards (Powers, 1973). They determine their match to standards through external feedback. Discrepancy reducing mechanisms are posited by control theory to operate between behavioral standards and external feedback. Individuals may also attempt to reduce discrepancies which exist between self-assessments of performance and external assessments of performance. This can be accomplished through the acceptance or rejection of external feedback. Lord and Hanges (1987) posit feedback acceptance as a boundary condition to a control theory model of the relation between goals and feedback. However, it may be that feedback acceptance is controlled at a lower level in the hierarchy of the control system operating to reduce the discrepancy between performance goal and performance feedback control. Control theory explanations of behavior are still applicable when considering the discrepancy between self-generated feedback and other generated feedback as that which behavior attempts to reduce. The perceived discrepancy between these two feedback sources will influence feedback acceptance and potentially cognitive and behavioral reaction to feedback. In control theory terms, self-feedback is the referent against which feedback from other sources is compared. Instead of concentrating on the regulation between a standard and feedback, the controlled 42 quantity in this utilization of control theory is the feedback itself. The following section describes a model which utilizes control theory to view the performance feedback process. External feedback is compared to self-feedback in this control system. The model details individual responses to incongruence between self and other feedback sources. Finally, this model will be used as a basis for advancing hypotheses regarding responses to feedback incongruence. CHAPTER 2: MODEL AND HYPOTHESBS Figure 5 represents a conceptual model illustrating the relationships among variables relevant to self-other feedback congruence. In line with a control theory perspective this model suggests that individuals regulate their behavior (box 2) in relation to a goal (box 1). In Campion and Lord’s control theory formulation, performance judgment or feedback from an external source (box 33) is perceived at the sensor (box 3b). A unique contribution of the control system model of self-other feedback congruence is the explicit addition of a self-generated performance judgment or self-feedback (box 4). The self-generated performance judgment can be thought of as a standard or referent in control theory terms. People strive to maintain a consistent view of themselves and often bias search for self-relevant information toward self- confirmation (Swarm & Read, 1981). Individuals often believe that their self-evaluations are the correct ones, therefore their attention to self- relevant information is biased. Feedback provided by an external source is compared with the referent of self-feedback through the comparator function (box 5). The feedback is compared in terms of discrepancy and sign of the feedback. If no discrepancy is detected (box 6) between the self-feedback and external feedback and external feedback is positive, feedback acceptance 43 Figure 5. Self-other feedback congruence model oweqnu wou>enom owaocu ~ouoa>ozen . uoq>ocom u=o«>uum cu :HJJuMII. IlmLtw acu>unon on xuanvoum cocaBMOuuom azmzzomH>ZM vuuouocoo aaaocuouxm I I I, I Asaumsmv q<=oH>HazH ul- IM~:II oz I! - oz uoa>ozon owcozu III 0“ cu coaucoucu sucaua«c~uu o Abouuov ouuaom a“ Auouuov mo* >ucoauuonwn no >ucoaouuuao n monogamou< an III. . sooavoum uonoom nuaueoEomvoh vououucoo n .eoecou\aooov aaaocuouxm - can»: IIII heuounnaco xumaewom zoeoaouomwvt aaficacomz III casoumcn Auunuo\CHomv ccwnauon \ououonuoucn neueueaaou q anacmzuoz . .III xuoavoum - neumfiuon ouceEhouuom . ucaaowvah IIII vououucuu GUGIIhOwHUAH mn HHQMI noncoecoo oueeunouu< xuoaeoom acuuouuu wououocuo m «mango uaom suacauuoo «Hum o>wuucwou , — IIII . H anon- ed uncozu ueouuuum Heoc Ill 45 will be more likely to result (box 7). If a discrepancy between the external and self-generated feedback is detected, feedback acceptance is less likely to occur. The decision of whether to incorporate external feedback or not (box 8) will be influenced by self-certainty (box 9) and source credibility (box 10). If self-certainty is high, acceptance of self-generated feedback (box 11) should be high. Similarly, if source credibility is high acceptance of externally generated feedback (box 7) should be high. The extent to which self-generated feedback (box 4) and externally generated feedback (box 33) influence consensus performance judgment (box 12) is a function ‘ of self-generated and externally generated feedback acceptance. Consensus performance judgment is a clinical combination of the information which is received from oneself and an external source of feedback. It is this consensus performance judgment that is compared to the goal through a comparator function (box 13) to determine the adequacy of performance. It is at this point where the controlled quantity changes. The system is no longer controlling for a self/other feedback discrepancy, but controls for a goal/consensus performance judgment feedback discrepancy. This comparison represents the traditional way control theory has been applied in organizations. A goal is compared to the feedback (consensus 46 performance judgment) to determine if a discrepancy (box 14) exists. If no discrepancy exists, previous behavior is continued. If a discrepancy exists, a decision mechanism (box 15) is engaged and the individual makes either a cognitive (goal change box 16) or behavioral change (intention to change behavior box 17). A complete understanding of the preceding formulation requires a detailed explanation of the proposed relationships among the variables which have been presented. The focus of this study is on the control theory formulation of self-other feedback discrepancy, because traditional control theory applications of goal/feedback discrepancy have been previously studied (e.g. See Campion & Lord, 1982; Hollenbeck & Williams, 1987; Klein, 1987). The following section provides descriptions of each of the components to be assessed. The unique contribution of each of the variables is discussed. Hypotheses regarding the proposed relationships among these variables are then presented. Goals and Performance Previous research has clearly established the positive relationship between goals and performance (Locke, et al., 1981). Goals which are specific and difficult direct behavior toward a desired outcome, such as task performance. 47 Feedback After a task has been performed, feedback is provided. Feedback is a message an individual receives from a source, which provides information regarding the adequacy of task performance (llgen et al. 1979). Feedback can originate from a variety of sources, the task, other individuals or from the individual him/herself (Greller & Herold, 1975). Self-generated feedback is a primary component in self-assessment. Theories of self-regulation (Bandura, 1977) suggest that individuals observe and evaluate their own behavior. Self-generated feedback is a personal judgment regarding performance. This self-evaluation of performance serves as a referent against which all other assessments of performance are compared. External feedback is the type of feedback which is generally discussed in the literature. It is feedback which is not intrinsic to the performer. External feedback can come from supervisors, co-worker, subordinates or from the task itself. Research has focused on interpersonal sources of feedback, specifically supervisory feedback. While feedback is always present in some form, all sources of feedback are not perceived equally. Self-feedback is the standard against which other forms of feedback are compared. Feedback which is not perceived as accurate will be rejected (llgen et al., 1979; Taylor et al., 1984). Feedback Discrepancy Feedback discrepancy refers to the lack of agreement among feedback sources. While much research has investigated the influence of various sources of feedback (organizational, self, supervisor, co-worker, subordinate and task), little research has addressed the effect of disagreement among feedback sources. Fisher & Russ (1987) discuss several reasons why self-superior feedback disagreements occur. Stone and Stone (1985) addressed the issue of whether consistent feedback from multiple sources is more effective in altering self-perceptions than the provision of consistent feedback on multiple occasions by a single feedback source. In addition, they investigated the effects of single versus multiple feedback sources on self-perceived task competence. However, no research has investigated the influence of agreement among the two most common feedback sources, self- and supervisory feedback. Agreement can be assessed in absolute terms and in terms of feedback sign. External feedback can be similar or dissimilar to self- feedback and can be above (positive sign) or below (negative sign) self- feedback. Both of these variables will influence feedback acceptance. 49 Eeedback Acceptance A variable identified by llgen et al. (1979) as an important mediator between objective feedback and performance is feedback acceptance. Feedback acceptance is defined as the perceived accuracy of the feedback. Whether or not the feedback is actually an accurate portrayal of performance is insignificant to feedback acceptance. Since feedback acceptance can be linked to key organizational outcomes, its study is important. Individuals are often unwilling to accept information about themselves which is inconsistent with what they know about themselves ' (Shrauger, 1975; Swarm & Read, 1982). Individuals tend to reject information that is inconsistent with their own self-view. Another aspect of acceptance which has not been discussed in the literature is acceptance of or perceived accuracy of self-generated feedback. Self-generated feedback derives from the performance judgment that one makes regarding his/her own performance. Therefore, the following hypotheses regarding factors affecting feedback acceptance are posited: H1: Feedback sign will be positively related to acceptance of externally generated feedback. H2: Feedback discrepancy will be negatively related to acceptance of externally generated feedback. 50 H3: There will be an interaction between feedback sign and feedback discrepancy on acceptance of external feedback, such that the relationship between feedback sign and feedback acceptance will be stronger when self-other discrepancy is high. Source Credibility A characteristic of the feedback source that can influence the relationship between objective feedback and feedback acceptance is the credrbility of the source. Source credibility is defined as the perceived expertise of the feedback source. While a direct relationship between source credibility and feedback acceptance has been established (see e.g. Halperin, Snyder, Shenkel & Houston, 1976; Stone, et al., 1984), no research has addressed the interactive effect of source credibility and feedback discrepancies. In addition, llgen et al. (1979) suggest that acceptance of external feedback that is negative will be moderated by source credibility. However, research to date has not investigated this issue. The present research hypothesizes that: H4: The relationship between the feedback comparator function (feedback discrepancy, feedback sign) and acceptance of externally generated feedback will be moderated by source credibility. The relationship will be stronger when source credibility is high. Seigcminu Self-certainty refers to the confidence one has in his/her self-view. When one is certain about possessing an attribute, the perceived accuracy of self-generated feedback should be high (Swarm & Read, 1981). 51 Markus (1977) compared schematics (those who had well developed self- views) to aschematics (those who did not have well developed self-views) on the attribute of assertiveness. She found that schematic individuals were likely to reject feedback that was inconsistent with their self-schema. Those who believed they were assertive rejected feedback indicating they were not assertive and those who believed they were unassertive rejected feedback indicating they were assertive. When self-certainty is low individuals accept information about themselves whether it be discrepant from their own self-view or not. This suggests that when individuals do not have a well established self-view they will be less likely to perceive their self-generated feedback as accurate, therefore acceptance of self-generated feedback should be diminished. New employees prefer feedback from others as opposed to self-generated feedback (Hillery & Wexley, 1974). Acceptance of self- relevant information is also dependent on its information value, i.e.; the incremental increase in knowledge about the self that the information provides (llgen et al., 1979). When people are not self-certain, self- generated feedback may be perceived as less valuable. Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed: H5: The relationship between the comparator (feedback discrepancy, feedback sign) and acceptance of self-generated feedback will be moderated by self-certainty. The relationship will be stronger when self-certainty is high. 52 Congnsus Performance Judgment Consensus performance judgment refers to a clinical combination of performance relevant feedback from varying sources. In this study, we are interested in the combination of feedback from oneself and feedback from an external source. The importance placed on these two sources of feedback will be influenced by the acceptance (perceived accuracy) of feedback from each source: H6: The relationship between self-generated feedback and consensus performance judgment will be moderated by acceptance of self- generated feedback. The relationship will be stronger when acceptance of self-generated feedback is high. H7:The relationship between externally generated feedback and consensus performance judgment will be moderated by acceptance of external feedback. This relationship will be stronger when acceptance of external feedback is high. No specific hypotheses are proposed to predict the interaction of self-and other generated feedback on consensus performance judgment. This relationship will be investigated in an exploratory fashion. No basis exists for predicting the weight that either self or other generated feedback will be given in such an analysis. CHAPTER 3: METHOD Prior to conducting the main study of this project it was necessary to conduct two pilot studies. These pilot studies addressed several key issues. This study was interested in assessing reactions to feedback discrepant from one’s own self-view. The provision of highly discrepant feedback requires that self-ratings are neither too low nor too high. Therefore, it was necessary to choose a task for the main study that would not result in either a self- assessment floor or ceiling effect. The primary purpose of Pilot Study I was to test two tasks and choose the one resulting in a moderate mean self-assessment rating. In addition, the strength of the credibility and feedback congruence manipulations were Checked. Finally, the reliabilities of some of the dependent measures were assessed in this pilot study. Due to the results of Pilot Study 1, a second pilot study was conducted. In this pilot study, the strength of the manipulations were checked and the reliabilities of the measures assessed. Pilot Stud Subjects Subjects in the first pilot study were 33 undergraduate students enrolled in an introductory psychology course Fall term, 1988. Students received course credit in return for their participation. 53 54 flask The first pilot study was conducted in a laboratory setting. Two tasks were developed for this study. The first task was a sales task in which subjects were asked to role play selling a nutritional supplement to a confederate. Subjects were provided with a detailed description of a fictitious nutritional supplement. They were also given information regarding the steps to an effective sale as well as fabricated testimonials from satisfied customers. They were given 15 minutes to familiarize themselves with the material and 15 minutes to sell the product to the confederate. The second task was an in-basket exercise. The in-basket exercise used in this study was developed for use in an assessment center for a large midwestem computer organization. The original in-basket was designed for salespeople who were allowed 75 minutes to complete it. The in-basket was shortened for the purposes of this study. Subjects were allowed 20 minutes to complete the in-basket exercise. This shortened version of the in-basket was piloted to determine the amount of time needed to complete the exercise. 11% Pilot study I subjects arrived at the experimental session individually. Subjects were escorted to the room where the experimental session was to take place. They were asked to read and sign an informed consent form. The task in which they were engaging (sales or in-basket) was then explained to them 55 and the rater credibility manipulation was given. This manipulation consisted of telling subjects they would be evaluated by either an undergraduate student who was also participating in the study for course credit or by a graduate student. Subjects were given 20 minutes to complete the task. After completion of the task the experimenter returned and asked the subject to fill out a self-assessment form and a self-certainty questionnaire (These questionnaires are described in greater detail in the section to follow titled "Variables and Measures"). These items were collected from the subject and a filler task consisting of attitude and personality questionnaires was given. In the meantime the rater ostensrhly rated the subjects’ performance. Subjects were shown the rater’s evaluation and asked to complete a feedback acceptance questionnaire. Subjects were debriefed and thanked for their participation. Banks The first pilot study was conducted before a determination was made to include both positive and negative feedback in the study. Therefore, the sales and in-basket tasks were pre-tested to determine which was least likely to result in a self-rating flpo; effect. A floor effect would prevent the possibility of providing feedback which was negatively discrepant from self-assessments. The sales task resulted in mean self-assessments of 4.3 (on a 10-point scale), while the average self-rating for in-basket exercise performance was 5.0. In addition, 56 subjects displayed a great deal of resistance to participating in the sales task. Therefore, the in-basket exercise was chosen as the task to be completed in this study. Comments from subjects who completed the in-basket exercise indicated that the time allowed to complete the exercise was insufficient. Therefore, it was decided that subjects would be allowed 45 minutes to complete the exercise rather than 20 minutes. The results of the rater credibility manipulation indicate that the manipulation was not strong enough. A chi-square statistics was calculated to be 10.51 df = 10 and the significance level was .40. Due to these results, the credrhility manipulation was made stronger in pilot study 11. Not only were subjects informed that the rater was either an undergraduate or industrial/organizational graduate student, but they were told that the undergraduate had absolutely no experience in rating performance while the graduate student had extensive experience in rating in-basket exercise performance. In addition, the manipulation was presented after completion of the task to make it more salient and to eliminate any influence the manipulation might have had on performance. The reliabilities of the feedback acceptance and the self-certainty scales were also assessed. It was determined that the feedback acceptance scale was reliable (alpha = .83; n=33) but the self-certainty scale needed slight 57 modifications (alpha = .72; n=33). Also, the variance on self-certainty was relatively low (Mean = 4.36 SD = .61). Pilot Studffll meets Subjects in the second pilot study were 33 undergraduate students enrolled in a psychology course winter term, 1988. Students received extra credit in return for their participation. Task The second pilot study was also conducted in a laboratory setting. The task subjects were asked to complete was an in—basket exercise. Subjects were allowed 45 minutes to complete the exercise. The in-basket was a modified version of one used to select salespeople at a large midwestern computer organization. Procedure Pilot study 11 subjects arrived at the experimental sessions individually. They were escorted to the room where the experimental session was to take place. Subjects were asked to read and sign an informed consent form. The in-basket exercise was then explained to them and they were allowed 45 minutes to complete the exercise. 58 After 45 minutes the experimenter collected the in-basket materials and asked the participants to complete a self-evaluation and self-certainty questionnaire (Questionnaires are described in greater detail in the section to follow titled "Variables and Measures"). At this time, the subjects were given the credibility manipulation. They were told that their performance was either being evaluated by an undergraduate student with no previous experience in performance assessment or in-basket exercises, 9; that they were being evaluated by an industrial/organizational psychology student with much experience in the evaluation of in-basket exercise performance. The participants were asked to complete an attitude questionnaire while the rater ostensibly rated their performance. The experimenter returned 15 minutes later and showed the participant the evaluation that the rater had provided. The ratings were manipulated to be 0, 10 or 20 points (based on pilot study I data) below the participants’ self- rating, depending on the feedback congruence condition to which the subject had been assigned. The subject was then asked to complete a questionnaire including the feedback acceptance, perceived credibility and perceived feedback congruence scales. Finally, they were asked to complete a second self- evaluation and indicate whether or not they intended to change their behavior in a second in-basket exercise. Subjects were debriefed, and thanked for their participation. 59 Basic The results of the manipulation checks on source credibility and feedback discrepancy, indicated that there were statistically significant differences in subjects perceptions of l) the credibility of the feedback source F(1,31)=12.39, p<.001 and 2) feedback congruence F(2,30)=27.48, p<.001. A post-hoe Scheffe test indicated that the high, moderate and low feedback congruence conditions were significantly different and in the expected direction (See Table 1). Another purpose of the second pilot study was to test the reliabilities of the dependent measures. Descriptive statistics, intercorrelations and reliabilities for these scales are provided in Table 2. It was determined that the reliabilities of the dependent variable scales were adequate. rima tud m Subjects were non-freshman undergraduate students enrolled in psychology courses in March and April of 1989. Subjects received course credit in return for their participation. A power analysis was conducted to determine the sample size needed with a statistical power of .80. The analysis requiring the greatest statistical power attempts to explain a variance of .4 and the effect size associated with the interaction term explains an incremental increase of at least 2.5% of the variance. The number of subjects needed to detect this 60 Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations for Manipulated Variables. DV Source Credibilitvfi (High Cred) (Low Cred) Mean SD Mean _S__12 Perceived Source Credibility 5.47 1.14 3.90 1.41 DV Feedback Discrepancy (High Disc) (Low Disc) (No Disc) Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Perceived Feedback Congruence 2.63 .97 4.95 1.19 6.30 1.38 'il.‘ “ -..‘7-. 61 Table 2. Descriptive Statistics, Intercorrelations and Reliabilities of Dependent Measures. DV Mean SD 1 2 3 4 1) Perceived Source Credibility 4.71 .49 (.89)* 2) Perceived Feedback Congruence 4.51 .92 .27 (.96) 3) Feedback Acceptance 4.31 .53 .39 .74 (.79) 4) Self Certainty 4.61 .10 -.01 -.17 -.14 (.79) "Values in parentheses indicate alpha reliability coefficients. Correlations less than -.28 or greater than .28 are significant at the .05 level. 62 effect with a desired statistical power of .80 at the .05 level was calculated to be 194. Data was collected from 280 subjects who were assigned-to one of three feedback discrepancy conditions (high, low, no), one of three feedback sign conditions (positive, neutral, negative), and one of two source credibility conditions (high, low). It was discovered that one of the research assistants participating in this project was providing subjects with incorrect ’7 instructions. It was therefore decided that the data from the 24 subjects who .. received the erroneous information would be deleted from the study. The final sample size was reduced to 256. The design appears to be a 3 x 3 x 2 factorial design but this is not the case. Feedback discrepancy is defined as feedback which is discrepant from one’s own self-view and feedback sign is defined as being above (positive), below (negative) or the same as (neutral) one’s self-rating. Therefore, whenever non-discrepant feedback was provided, this feedback was also neutral in sign. Similarly, feedback discrepant from self-ratings could not have a neutral sign (See Figure 6 for the experimental design). Due to an error in subject assignments to experimental conditions, data collection occurred in two stages. The first group of subjects were randomly assigned to one of six conditions. These were conditions 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, and 10 as depicted in Figure 6. Subjects in the second group were then randomly 63 assigned to the remaining four conditions. These were conditions 2, 4, 6, and 8 (See Figure 6). To assess the similarity between subjects in the first and second phases of the study, comparisons were made between these two subject groups on individual difference variables of self-esteem, need for achievement, need for autonomy, need for affiliation, need for dominance and locus of control. Subjects in these two groups did not significantly differ on any of these individual difference variables. Task The task used in this study was the in-basket exercise described above (See Appendix A). An in-basket exercise represents a simulation in which subjects are presented with a variety of in-basket items (memos, reports and notes) which might be found in a typical manager’s in-basket. It has been suggested that the skills subjects display in analyzing and evaluating the data is representative of how they will resolve similar tasks on the job (Bray & Grant, 1966). Procedure Upon arrival at the experimental session, subjects were asked to read and sign an informed consent declaration (See Appendix B for measures used in this study). Subjects were read a preliminary script that introduced the task. Figure 6. Experimental Design. Feedback Sign High Credibility Low Credibility Feedback Sign Feedback Discrepancy Condition High Low None 1 3 n=26 n=29 5 n=29 2 4 n=22 n=22 6 8 n=23 n=22 10 n=27 7 9 n=28 n=28 65 The in-basket evaluation procedure and an explanation of the performance dimensions to be used for evaluation was then explained to subjects. Subjects were given 45 minutes to complete the in-basket exercise. When time had expired, they were asked to assess their own performance and to complete the self-certainty and acceptance of self-generated feedback measures both of which are described below. The self-assessment and self-certainty/self-generated feedback acceptance questionnaire were collected and subjects were given the credibility manipulation (See Appendix C) and asked to complete a series of .. personality questionnaires while the rater ostensibly evaluated their performance. Subjects were given false feedback regarding their performance according to the feedback discrepancy and feedback sign conditions to which they were assigned. Subjects then completed a questionnaire assessing their reactions to the feedback. This measure included the external feedback acceptance perceived credibility of the feedback source and perceived feedback congruence scales. Subjects were then told they would be completing a second in-basket exercise. They were asked to again assess their own performance after careful consideration of the previously completed self- and rater’s evaluation of performance. Subjects were debriefed and thanked for their participation. Variables and Measures Provided below is a detailed description of the variables which were assessed in this study. See Appendix B for the actual measures which were used. Feedback Sigp. Feedback sign was operationalized in a way that is unique to this study. Traditional performance feedback studies have used a goal as the referent against which the evaluator’s performance feedback is compared. However in this study, the referent against which the evaluator’s performance feedback is compared is self-feedback n_o_t a performance goal. Sign of the feedback is defined as whether the feedback provided by the evaluator is above, below or equal to the referent of self-feedback. Feedback sign was manipulated in this study resulting in three levels of feedback sign (positive, negative, neutral). Positive feedback was above the referent, negative feedback was below the referent and neutral feedback was exactly equal to the referent of self-feedback. Feedback Discrepang. Feedback discrepancy was defined as the degree of disagreement between one’s self-assessment and an evaluator’s external assessment of performance. This variable was manipulated resulting in three levels of feedback discrepancy (high, low, no). In the high feedback discrepancy condition, the evaluator’s external assessment was 20 points above or below the subject’s self-assessment. In the low feedback discrepancy 67 condition the evaluator-’s external assessment was 10 points above or below the subject’s self-evaluation. The point values for the high and low discrepancy conditions were based on pilot study data. These values represented feedback that was approximately two standard deviations away from the average self-assessment in the high feedback discrepancy condition, or approximately one standard deviation away 1 1 £135.- ::0.r.qnu-ou ‘ p from the average self-assessment in the low feedback discrepancy condition. In the no feedback discrepancy condition the evaluator’s external I. assessment exactly matched the subject’s self-assessment. This discrepancy condition is confounded with the neutral feedback sign condition. When the exialuator’s feedback matches self-feedback (no discrepancy), feedback sign MUST be neutral (See Figure 6). Perceived Feedback Congr_uence. The perceived degree of congruence between self- and external assessments of performance, was assessed with a self-report measure developed specifically for this study. This measure assessed the effectiveness of the discrepancy manipulation. A sample item from this scale is, ’The evaluation 1 received was similar to my self-evaluation’. Subjects were asked to indicate, on a 1 - 7 scale, the extent to which they agreed with the statements presented. Source Credibility. Source credibility has been defined as the rater’s expertise in evaluating performance. This variable was manipulated resulting in 68 two levels of source credibility (high, low). In the high credibility condition, subjects were told they were being evaluated by an industrial/organizational psychology graduate student with 3 years of previous experience in evaluating in-basket exercises. In the low credibility condition, subjects were told they were being evaluated by an undergraduate student also participating in the study for course credit, with no experience with in-basket exercise evaluation. See Appendix C for the complete script of the source credibility manipulation. Perceived Source Credibilig. The perceived source credibility was assessed with a self-report measure. This measure was used to determine the effectiveness of the credibility manipulation. A sample item from this scale is, The rater who evaluated my performance was knowledgeable’. Subjects were asked to indicate, on a 1 - 7 scale, the extent to which they agreed with the statements presented. Self-Rating. Subjects were asked to provide one overall rating of their in- basket exercise performance. They were asked to recall the performance goal of 70, prior to making their self-ratings. The form subjects used to evaluate their performance appears in Appendix B. External Rating. False feedback from an external rater was provided to subjects. This feedback provided one overall rating of the subjects’ in-basket exercise performance. The rating was provided on a form that was essentially identical to the self-rating form (See Appendix B). 69 Consensus Performance Judgr_n_ent. Subjects were led to believe they would be participating in second in-basket exercise. Prior to completing this exercise they were asked to complete a second self-assessment measure. This measure was identical to the first except, subjects were asked to consider both their self-evaluation of performance as well as the rater’s evaluation of their performance in providing a second assessment of their previous performance. The weight subjects assigned to either their self-rating or the external rating was left to them. @nemal Feedback Acceptance. External feedback acceptance is the perceived accuracy of the feedback provided by an external source. This variable was assessed with a modified version of the feedback acceptance measure used by Stone, Guetal and McIntosh (1984). A sample item from this scale is ’The feedback did not truly depict my performance on the sales task.’ Subjects were asked to indicate, on a 1 - 7 scale, the extent to which they agreed with the statements presented. Self-Feedback Acceptance. Acceptance of performance feedback provided to oneself was assessed in a manner similar to acceptance of externally generated feedback. The same measure was used but subjects were asked to consider the feedback they provided for themselves when they rated the perceived accuracy of the feedback. One item from the external feedback acceptance scale had to be eliminated from the self-feedback acceptance scale 70 because when the item was converted from acceptance of another’s feedback to acceptance of self-feedback it no longer made sense. Subjects were asked to indicate, on a 1 - 7 scale, the extent to which they agreed with the statements presented. Self-Certainty. Self-certainty refers to the faith one has in his/her self- evaluation of performance. This variable was assessed by a scale developed for the purposes of this study. A sample item from this scale is, ’I am confident about the self-evaluation I provided.’ Subjects were asked to indicate, on a 1 - I. 7 scale, the extent to which they agreed with the statements presented. Self-Confidence. The self-certainty and the self-feedback acceptance séales were so highly correlated that a decision was made to combine the two scales and call it self-confidence. The basis for this combination is described in detail in the results section. CHAPTER 4: RESULTS Prior to testing the proposed hypotheses, descriptive statistics were calculated for the variables of interest in this study. The means, standard deviations and reliabilities for the variables assessed in this study are provided in Table 3. First, these data reveal a mean self-rating of approximately 58 with a standard deviation of approximately 15. This indicates that there was neither a floor nor a ceiling effect for self-ratings of performance. Perceived rater expertise was strongly correlated with manipulated rater expertise. Similarly, perceived congruence between self- and external ratings of performance was strongly negatively correlated with the manipulated discrepancy between these two ratings. Note the strong correlation between self-certainty and self-feedback acceptance. Both self-certainty and self-feedback acceptance were measured using a single questionnaire. Due to the strong correlation between these scales, a factor analysis was completed for this questionnaire. The rotated factor loadings indicated that the two factors extracted, with eigenvalues greater than one, corresponded to one factor for positively worded items and one for negatively worded items (See Appendix D). Upon examination of the items from the two scales, it was determined that they were quite similar. An examination of the intercorrelations between the items of these scales also 71 '72! Table 3. Descriptive Statistics, Intercorrelations and Reliabilities of Measures. !gge leer 50 1 2 3 1 5 6 1 8 9 10 ll 12 11 Feedback Sign 1.00 0.88 21 leedbecl lircrepercy 2. H 6 0.16 -.006 31 Perceived leedbtcl Congruence 1.56 1.68 .153 -.185 (.921 11 Source Credibility 1.50 0.50 .097 -.026 .036 51 Perceived Source Credibility 1.16 1.11 .261 -.011 .218 .581 1.8311 61 Sell-Rating 58.36 11.16 .032 -.050 .091 .103 .155 11 Irterrtl Rating 58.32 20.51 .669 -.016 .168 .133 .211 .133 81 Correrrur irri. lldxeent 59.81 16.51 .301 -.018 .153 .113 .227 .870 .811 91 External Feedback Acceptance 1.50 1.38 .383 -.353 .506 .185 .116 .212 .118 .283 1.861 101 Sell-Feedback lccepterce 1.53 1.21 -.005 -.013 .139 .015 .059 .362 . V“. ‘8 ' .351 .105 1.951 11) Self-Certeitty 1.11 1.15 .011 .000 .111 -.002 .092 .393 .321 .361 .299 .615 (.19; 12} Sell- Coriidelce 1.50 1.09 .021 -.016 .151 .018 .086 .111 .335 .392 .371 .8’3 .919 1.8:? 38!]!98 in parentheses indicate tlplt reliebility COEIIICICDIS. Correlations (renter than .100 are titlilicert it the p<.05 level (n=256). 73 indicated a significant correspondence between these two scales. The scales were therefore combined resulting in one scale representing self-confidence in self-ratings. The reliability coefficient for this combined scale was .86 which was higher than the reliability for either of the scales separately (See Table 3). Self-ratings were positively correlated with self-certainty, self-feedback acceptance and with the combined self-confidence scale. This indicates that the higher subjects self-ratings, the higher their faith or confidence in those ratings. Alpha reliability coefficients were calculated for scales used in this study when possrhle. The coefficients for the scales measured were well within acceptable ranges (.75 - .92). Manipulation Check_s Descriptive statistics were analyzed to determine the effectiveness of the source credibility and feedback discrepancy manipulations. First, the correlation between source credibility and perceived source credibility was positive and significant (r=.587). This correlation indicates that high credibility sources of feedback were perceived as significantly more expert than low credibility sources of feedback. Note, however that subjects in the low credibility condition still perceived their feedback source as somewhat credible. The mean perceived credibility rating for the low credibility group was 4.08 on a 7- point scale (See Table 4). 74 Table 4. Means and standard deviations for manipulated variables. Dependent Variable = Perceived Source Credibility Source Credibility (High Cred) Mean SD 5.44 .94 (Low Cred) Mean S_Q 4.08 .95 Dependent Variable= Perceived Feedback Congruence Feedback Discrepancy (High Disc) Mean 3.22 _S__I2 1.09 (Low Disc) (No Disc) Mean 4.73 S12 1.18 M 6.74 _S__12 .54 . -. c L...“ 75 A one-way ANOVA was also computed to determine the effectiveness of the feedback discrepancy manipulation. Manipulated feedback discrepancy (high, low, no) served as the independent variable and perceived congruence between self- and external feedback served as the dependent variable. This analysis revealed statistically significant differences in subject’s perceptions of congruence between self- and external performance feedback F(2,252)=206.38, p<.001 dependent upon the discrepancy condition to which they had been assigned. A post hoc Scheffe test indicated that all three feedback discrepancy conditions were significantly different from each other (See Table 4). Hypotheses thgpothesis 1 The first hypothesis posited a positive relationship between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance. A zero-order correlation coefficient was calculated between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance to test this hypothesis. In support of the hypothesis, the correlation coefficient between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance was positive and statistically significant (r = .383, p<.001). To determine the mean differences which occurred in external feedback acceptance at positive, negative and neutral sign levels, a post-hoc Scheffe test was conducted. The result of this analysis indicated, as expected, positive feedback was more accepted than negative feedback and neutral feedback 76 (feedback identical to self-ratings) was more accepted than negative feedback. A significant difference in external feedback acceptance was not found between positive and neutral feedback. Descriptive statistics for this analysis appear in Table 5 and the relationship between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance is portrayed in Figure 7. Hypothesis 2 The second hypothesis suggested a negative relationship would exist between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance. The zero order correlation coefficient between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance was negative and significant (r = -.353, p<.001). A partial correlation coefficient was also calculated between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance, controlling for the effects of feedback sign. The partial correlation coefficient between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance partialling out feedback sign was r = -.383 (p<.001). This partial correlation indicates that feedback discrepancy had a unique influence on external feedback acceptance, over and above the influence of feedback sign. A post-hoe Scheffe test was completed to determine mean differences in external feedback acceptance at the three feedback discrepancy levels (none, low, high). This test indicated that external feedback acceptance significantly differed across all three feedback discrepancy conditions. That is, 77 Table 5. Means, standard deviations and cell sizes for external feedback acceptance by feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and source credrhility. External Feedback Acceptance Independent Variable Mean 82 N External Feedback Sign . Positive 4.87 1.10 100 Neutral 5.32 1.37 56 Negative 3.68 1.22 100 External Feedback Discrepancy High 4.01 1.40 99 Low 4.54 1.14 101 None 5.32 1.37 56 Source Credibility High 4.76 1.37 128 Low 4.25 1.35 128 78 Figure 7. Plot of relationship between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance. . Q&A.-‘_l“h‘l'g-m . . . . A External Feedback Acceptance Negative Neutral Positive Feedback Sign 79 non-discrepant external feedback was significantly more accepted than feedback which was slightly discrepant from self-feedback, which was in turn significantly more accepted than highly discrepant feedback (See Table 5). The relationship between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance is illustrated in Figure 8. Eco—“1&3 Hypothesis three stated that feedback discrepancy would moderate the relationship between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance. To test this hypothesis, moderated regression analysis was used. The analysis involved regressing external feedback acceptance on a) feedback sign, b) feedback discrepancy and c) an interaction of feedback sign x feedback discrepancy. Feedback sign was entered first followed by feedback discrepancy. The cross- product of these two terms was then entered. Table 6 shows the standardized regression coefficients and significance levels for each of the independent variables in the equation. The results revealed an overall R-squared for external feedback acceptance of .277, F(3,252) = 32.18, p<.001. Hypothesis three stated that feedback discrepancy would moderate the relationship between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance. However, as can be seen in Table 6, the regression coefficient representing the interaction of feedback sign and feedback discrepancy was not significant. Post-hoc analyses confirmed these results. The plot of the 80 Figure 8. Plot of the relationship between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance. External Feedback Acceptance Negative Neutral Positive Feedback Sign 81 Table 6. Regression of External Feedback Acceptance on Feedback Sign and Feedback Discrepancy. Independent Beta Multiple R-Square R-Square Significance Variable R Change of Change 1* Feedback Sign .3829 .3829 .1466 .1466 .000 3. Feedback Discrepancy .3511 .5195 .2699 .1233 .000 9 Feedback Sign x Feedback Discrepancy .4513 .5263 .2770 .0071 .117 N=256 82 relationship between feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance appears in Figure 9. Hypothesis 4 Hypothesis four suggested that the relationship between the comparator (feedback discrepancy and feedback sign) and external feedback acceptance would be moderated by source credibility. Moderated regression analysis was used in which feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and source credibility were entered into the regression equation. The cross products of feedback sign x feedback discrepancy, feedback sign x source credibility, feedback discrepancy x sourceicredibility and feedback sign x feedback discrepancy x source credibility were then entered respectively. Results revealed an overall R-squared for external feedback acceptance of .301, F(7, 248) = 15.29 p< .001. No significant interactions were found. Main effects for the independent variables of feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and source credibility were found. Table 7 shows the standardized regression coefficients and significance levels of each of the independent variables in the equafion. These findings suggest that in addition to feedback sign and feedback discrepancy, source credibility made a significant independent contribution to the prediction of external feedback acceptance. The main effect for source credibility indicates that high credibility sources were associated with higher 83 Figure 9. Plot of the relationship between feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance.‘ External Feedback 4 Acceptance Low High Feedback Discrepancy "When feedback sign and feedback discrepancy are crossed, subjects in the no discrepancy/no feedback conditions are eliminated. This is because sign and discrepancy are completely confounded in these conditions. 84 Table 7. Regression of External Feedback Acceptance on Feedback Sign, Source Credibility and Feedback Discrepancy. Independent Beta Multiple R-Square R-Square Significance Variable R Change of Change Feedback Sign .3829 .3829 .1466 .1466 .000 Feedback Discrepancy .3511 .5195 .2699 .1233 .000 Source Credibility .1401 .5379 .2893 .0194 .009 Sign x Discrepancy .4640 .5448 .2968 .0075 .103 Sign x Credibility .0727 .5452 .2972 .0004 .693 Discrepancy x Credibility .1083 .5458 .2979 .0007 .630 Sign x Discrepancy x Credibility 1.0749 .5490 .3014 .0035 .264 N=256 1 85 levels of external feedback acceptance than low credibility sources. Plots of the relationship between feedback sign, feedback discrepancy, source credibility and external feedback acceptance appear in Figure 10. Exam; Hypothesis five suggested that the relationship between the comparator and self-feedback acceptance would be moderated by self-certainty. The self- certainty and self-feedback acceptance scales were combined, therefore this hypothesis was not tested. However, a test of the moderating effect of self- confidence on the relationship between the comparator and external feedback acceptance was conducted. Moderated regression analyses were used in which feedback sign, feedback discrepancy, self-confidence and their interactions were regressed on external feedback acceptance. This regression analysis revealed significant main effects for each of the independent variables of feedback sign, feedback discrepancy and self- confidence. The main effects for sign and discrepancy have already been discussed. The main effect for self-confidence suggests a positive relationship between self-confidence and external feedback acceptance such that when self- confidence is high, external feedback acceptance is higher than when self- confidence is low. In partial support for Hypothesis 5, a significant feedback discrepancy x self-confidence interaction on external feedback acceptance was found. Regression coefficients for this analysis appear in Table 8. 86 Figure 10. Plots of the relationship between feedback sign, feedback discrepancy, source credibility and external feedback acceptance. Low Credibility 7 Positive 6 5 External 4 Feedback Acceptance3 2 Negative 1 Sign Low High Feedback Discrepancy High Credibility 7 Positive Sign 6 X=5.00 X=5.06 5 (n :29) (n—‘26) External 4 X=4.33 Feedback (n=22) =3.52 Acceptance3 (n=22) 2 Negative Sign 1 Low High Feedback Discrepancy 87 Table 8. Regression of External Feedback Acceptance on Feedback Sign, Feedback Discrepancy and Self-Confidence. Independent Beta Multiple R-Square R-Square Significance Variable R Change of Change Feedback Sign .3829 .3829 .1466 .1466 .000 Feedback Discrepancy .3511 .5195 .2699 .1233 .000 Self-Confidence .3549 .6290 .3957 .1258 .000 Sign x Discrepancy .2857 .6313 .3985 .0028 .279 Sign x Self- Confidence .3503 .6362 .4047 .0062 .108 Discrepancy x Self-Confidence .7590 .6522 .4253 .0206 .003 Sign x Discrepancy x Credibility 1.8094 .6562 .4306 .0053 .129 N=256 88 Post-hoc analyses were completed to determine mean differences which occurred in external feedback acceptance when feedback discrepancy was crossed with self-confidence. These analyses revealed mean differences between high and no, and low and no feedback discrepancy groups when self- confidence was high. However, no mean differences occurred in external feedback acceptance when self-confidence was low. Figure 11 portrays the interaction of self-confidence and feedback discrepancy on external feedback acceptance. Hypothesis 6 Hypothesis six stated that an interaction would exist between self- feedback and self-feedback acceptance on consensus performance judgment. Since self-feedback acceptance and self-certainty were combined into a self- confidence scale, self-confidence was used as the second independent variable. To test the hypothesis that self-feedback and self-confidence would interact to influence consensus performance judgment, moderated regression analysis was used. Self-feedback and self-confidence were entered, followed by their cross product with consensus performance judgment serving as the criterion. A main effect for self-rating on consensus performance judgment was found. However, neither a main effect for self-confidence nor an interaction of self-rating and self-confidence was revealed. The beta weight for self-rating was positive, indicating a positive relationship between an individual’s self-rating and 89 Figure 11. Plot of the interaction of self-confidence and feedback discrepancy on external feedback acceptance. 7 6 5 External Feedback 4 High Self- Confidence* A 3 X=3.25 (n=13) Low Self- Confidence N 0 Low High Discrepancy Discrepancy Discrepancy Feedback Discrepancy *Low self-confidence is defined as one standard deviation below the mean self- confidence rating. High self-confidence is defined as one standard deviation above the mean self-confidence rating. 90 the consensus performance judgment. That is, when self-rating was high consensus performance judgment tended to be high. The beta weights for self- confidence and the cross product term representing the interaction between self-rating and self-confidence were not significant (See Table 9). Figure 12 provides a representation of the relationship between self-rating, self-confidence and consensus performance judgment. Hypothesis 7 Hypothesis seven suggested that external feedback and externally generated feedback acceptance would interact to affect consensus performance judgment. To test this hypothesis, moderated regression analysis was used. External feedback and externally generated feedback acceptance were entered with consensus performance judgment serving as the criterion. In the third step the cross product of external feedback and externally generated feedback acceptance was entered. The data from the present study support the prediction. A significant main effect for external feedback on consensus performance judgment was revealed. The beta weight for the external rating was positive indicating a positive relationship between external rating and consensus performance judgment. That is, when external rating was high, consensus performance judgment tended to be high. 91 Table 9. Regression of Consensus Performance Judgment on Self- Rating and Self-Confidence. Independent Beta Multiple R-Square R-Square Significance Variable R Change of Change 1‘ Self-Rating .8703 .8703 .7575 .7575 .000 .f Self-Confidence .0385 .8710 .7587 .0012 .258 Self-Confidence X Self-Rating .0064 .8710 .7587 .0000 .976 N=256 92 Figure 12. Plot of the relationship between self-rating, self-confidence and consensus performance judgment. Consensus Performance Judgment 100 High" Self- 90 Confidence 80 Low Self- Confidence 70 50 40 30 20 10 Low High Self-Performance Rating" " High self-confidence is defined as one standard deviation above the mean self-confidence rating. Low self-confidence is defined as one standard deviation below the mean self-confidence rating. "High self-rating is defined as one standard deviation above the mean self-rating. Low self-rating is defined as one standard deviation below the mean self-rating. 93 In addition, a significant main effect was found for external feedback acceptance on consensus performance judgment. The beta weight for the external feedback acceptance was positive indicating a positive relationship between external feedback acceptance and consensus performance judgment. That is, when external feedback acceptance was high, consensus performance judgment tended to be high. Finally, a significant interaction was found between external rating and external feedback acceptance on consensus performance judgment. Table 10 presents the regression coefficients for this analysis. Post-hoe comparisons revealed significant differences occurred in censensus performance judgment between high and low external ratings, when external feedback acceptance was high but not when it was low. Figure 13 displays the interaction between external feedback and external feedback acceptance on consensus performance judgment. Additional Resultjs Exploratory analyses were conducted to determine the relative influence of self-feedback and external feedback in predicting consensus performance judgment. A hierarchical regression analysis was completed to determine the results. Self-feedback and external feedback were allowed to enter the regression equation based on which variable contributed the most variance to the prediction of consensus performance judgment. Results revealed that self- 94 Table 10. Regression of Consensus Performance Judgment on External Rating and External Feedback Acceptance. Independent Beta Multiple R-Square R-Square Significance Variable R Change of Change External Feedback .8413 .8413 .7077 .7077 .000 External Feedback Acceptance .0827 .8446 .7134 .0056 .027 External Feedback x External Feedback Acceptance .6669 .8548 .7307 .0173 .000 N=256 95 Figure 13. Plot of the interaction between external feedback and external feedback acceptance on consensus performance judgment. Consensus Performance 50 Judgment 30 20 10 Low High External Performance Rating" High“ External Feedback Acceptance Low External Feedback Acceptance "‘ High external feedback acceptance is defined as one standard deviation above the mean external feedback acceptance rating. Low external feedback acceptance is defined as one standard deviation below the mean external feedback acceptance rating. "High external rating is defined as one standard deviation above the mean external rating. Low external rating is defined as one standard deviation below the mean external rating. 96 feedback entered the equation first, accounting for more than 75% of the variance in consensus performance judgment. However, external feedback did account for a significant amount of additional variance in consensus performance judgment. (See Table 11). 97 Table 11. Regression of Consensus Performance Judgment on Self- and External Ratings of Performance. Independent Beta Multiple R-Square R-Square Significance Variable R Change of Change Self-Rating .8703 .8703 .758 .758 .000 External Feedback .4396 .9203 .847 .090 .000 N=256 CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of discrepancies between self-assessments of performance and other assessments of performance on performance feedback acceptance. A model of self-other feedback congruence was proposed within a control theory framework. Hypotheses derived from this model were tested and partial support for the model was found. The model of self-other feedback congruence suggested that feedback from an external source is compared to self-feedback on the dimensions of feedback sign and feedback discrepancy. The results of the present study revealed that both the direction of the feedback discrepancy (sign) and the magnitude of the discrepancy influenced acceptance of feedback provided by an external source. The first two hypotheses that feedback discrepancy would be negatively related to external feedback acceptance, and feedback sign would be positively related to external feedback acceptance, were supported. The third hypothesis that feedback discrepancy and feedback sign would interact to influence external feedback acceptance was not supported. The model proposed that source credibility would play an important role in the decision to accept or reject feedback provided by an external source. Further, source credibility was hypothesized to interact with feedback sign and feedback discrepancy to influence external feedback acceptance. A direct 98 99 relationship was revealed between perceived credibility of the feedback source and acceptance of externally generated feedback. However, source credibility did not moderate either the relationship between feedback sign and external feedback acceptance or feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance. The model further suggested that self-certainty would influence the decision to accept or discount feedback provided to oneself. The hypotheses regarding the influence of self-certainty and that of self-generated feedback acceptance were modified. This change was necessary due to the strong relationship between these two variables. In retrospect, the constructs of self- certainty and self-feedback acceptance both refer to faith or confidence in one’s own assessment of performance. These variables were therefore combined into one scale called self-confidence. The first modified hypothesis proposed that the relationship between the comparator (feedback sign, feedback discrepancy) and external feedback acceptance would be moderated by self-confidence. Findings demonstrated that in partial support of this hypothesis, self-confidence modified the relationship between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance. Individuals high in self-confidence accepted external feedback which was not discrepant from their self-view significantly more than feedback which was highly discrepant. However, no significant mean differences in external feedback 100 acceptance occurred for those who were less confident in their self-ratings at any level of discrepancy. This finding suggests that when individuals have relatively little confidence in self-ratings, they may view themselves as low credIhility sources of feedback. When external sources of feedback agree with individual self-assessments, external sources of feedback are perceived as less credible. Therefore, externally-generated feedback is less accepted. In essence, the comparison between self-feedback and external feedback is less meaningful for individuals who lack confidence in their self-assessments. The final group of hypotheses derived from the model of self-other feedback congruence relate to the final judgment individuals make regarding their performance. The model suggested that the relationship between self- feedback and consensus performance judgment would be moderated by self- confidence. This hypothesis was not supported. Self-feedback was only found to have a direct influence on consensus performance judgment. A parallel hypothesis indicating that external feedback acceptance would moderate the relationship between external feedback and consensus performance judgment was supported. The model suggested that self- and external feedback would both contribute to the consensus judgment made regarding performance. However, no specific hypotheses regarding the relative weight given to these factors could 101 be gleaned from the model. Therefore, exploratory analyses were conducted to determine the relative influence self-generated feedback and externally generated feedback had on consensus performance judgment. Results revealed that self-feedback contributed to the variance in consensus performance judgment more than externally generated feedback. However, external feedback made a significant contribution to the variance in consensus performance judgment above the influence of self-feedback. The findings of this study are discussed in more detail below. First, results relevant to the first part of the model identifying determinants of feedback acceptance are elaborated. Results related to the second part of the model identifying determinants of consensus performance judgments are then discussed. A modified model of self-other feedback congruence is presented. Implications of this investigation for application in organizations as well as for future research are detailed. Finally, limitations to this inquiry are outlined. Determinants of Feedback Acceptance llgen et al. (1979) describe the concept of feedback sign as feedback indicating that performance was either above or below some standard. A unique contribution of this study to the performance feedback literature is it’s atypical operationalization of feedback sign. What makes the definition of feedback sign distinct is that the standard of comparison in this study was one’s own self-assessment of performance M an externally set goal. So while the 102 finding that positive feedback leads to higher levels of external feedback acceptance is consistent with previous literature (e.g. DeNisi, et al., 1983; Stone & Stone, 1984; Stone & Stone, 1985), it also provides additional information that previous studies do not. The present results suggest that no matter if the standard of comparison is defined as a goal or as one’s self-assessment of performance, sign is positively related to external feedback acceptance. One might suggest that feedback above the standard of self-feedback is generally positive and therefore more accepted than feedback below self-feedback. However, the average self-rating in this study was 58.36 when the assigned goal was 70. So, feedback which was slightly discrepant (10 points above self-rating) and in the positive direction, could still be considered negative in the traditional sense. This finding adds to the generalizability of the proposal that feedback indicating performance is above a standard leads to higher levels of feedback acceptance than feedback indicating one’s performance is below standard. The findings of this study are also consistent with previous work which found that the credibility of the feedback source directly influenced reactions to performance feedback (Bannister, 1986; Halperin et a], 1976; Stone et. a1, 1984, Podaskoff & Fahr, in press). The results show that when feedback sources were perceived as more credible, greater acceptance of the feedback provided by these sources occurred. An interesting yet unexpected finding was that source credibility did not interact with feedback sign to influence acceptance of 103 externally generated feedback. llgen et al. (1979) have suggested that negative feedback from a credible source would be more accepted than negative feedback from a less credible source. Our findings do not support this proposal however, this could have partly been due to the manipulation of credibility in this study. While high credibility sources were perceived as significantly more credible than low credibility sources of feedback, both feedback sources were viewed as credible. That is, the average perceived credibility rating was above the midpoint of the scale for both the high and low credibility groups. In addition, restriction of range occurred on the source credibility scale which was used. Perceived source credibility was assessed using a 7-point scale. The standard deviation of perceived source credibility at both high and low credibility was just less than one. This may have limited the possrhility of finding significant relationships. These findings regarding source credibility ar_e consistent with that of Podaskoff and Fahr (in press) who found no sign x credibility effect on feedback acceptance but did find a sign x credibility effect on performance. This study also investigated the influence of the magnitude of self-other feedback discrepancies on acceptance of externally generated feedback. The results clearly demonstrate that the extent to which performance feedback from an external source is incongruent with self-generated feedback regarding 104 performance influences acceptance of external feedback. The negative relationship between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance supports the prediction that more discrepant external feedback is less likely to be accepted than feedback similar to self-feedback. This finding supports the view that individuals prefer self-relevant information which is consistent with self-assessments (Korman, 1970; Shrauger, 1975). Even after the influence of feedback sign was partialled out, the negative relationship between feedback discrepancy and acceptance of external feedback remained strong. These results appear to counter the self- enhancement view of work motivation (Baumeister, 1982) which suggests that individuals prefer to see themselves in the most favorable light. However, these findings may in part be due to the unique operationalization of feedback sign used in this study. Even when sign was negative, it may not have threatened self-esteem, therefore the results do not necessarily contradict the self-enhancement view. Influences on Consensus Performance Judgment The relationship between feedback from an external source and one’s judgment about performance was moderated by the acceptance of externally generated feedback. This finding directly addresses the significance of feedback acceptance in the performance feedback process. In support of propositions by llgen et a1. (1979), feedback acceptance played an integral role in the 105 relationship between externally generated feedback and one’s final assessment of the adequacy of performance. It seems likely that feedback acceptance would also be important to an individual’s motivation to change his/her behavior based on the feedback that was provided. Feedback provided to oneself about the adequacy of performance was also found to significantly influence one’s overall evaluation of performance. This finding is consistent with work by Podaskoff and Fahr (in press) which suggests that self-processes are important to the performance feedback process. In addition, this finding supports previous work by Greller and his associates (Greller, 1980; Greller & Herold, 1975; Herold et al., 1987) indicating that feedback provided to oneself is perceived to be the most available and useful source of feedback. This proposition is further supported by the finding in the present research that self-feedback contributed more to one’s overall assessment of performance than external feedback did. These results are consistent with recent work by Barley (1988) which revealed that feedback from psychologically closer sources was more acceptable and was more likely to influence later performance than feedback from psychologically distant sources. In summary, the results of this study provide some support for the model of self-other feedback congruence. The results suggest that self-assessments are an important component of the performance feedback process. Self-evaluations serve as a standard against which feedback from external sources is compared. 106 These comparisons were found to influence the feedback recipient’s determination of feedback acceptance. One unexpected yet interesting finding was that self-feedback regarding the adequacy of performance influenced the overall judgment of performance made, regardless of self-confidence in those ratings. This finding supports the view that self-feedback represents a potent standard that is important to individuals. On the other hand, the findings indicate that feedback from an external source must be accepted before it is incorporated into one’s overall judgment of performance. Therefore, confidence or faith in the ratings provided appears to be an important factor when the feedback source is external but not when it is internal. Finally, self-generated feedback was shown to influence consensus performance judgment more than external feedback. This outcome provided further support that while external feedback influenced final assessments, self- evaluation plays an essential role in the performance feedback process. The model of self-other feedback congruence proposed in this study has been revised (See Figure 14). The changes that have been made in this model do not completely reflect the results of this study. Methodological difficulties occurred in the measurement of a few variables, therefore, it was not considered appropriate to modify the model to reflect the results relevant to these variables. Revisions to the self-other model of feedback congruence are 107 Figure 14. Modified Model of Self-Other Feedback Congruence. ownenu . owaesu uow>unom 3328 ~33 Sufism scented“ ouoeehouuom quzzomw>zu voueuoeoc uoaauauQEIIII- .IIIIIIIIIIIII .IIIIIIII IIILuummmwmwwmlelIlll I omoenu ou nowuuuuou - oz AHQZH — oz 2; 1838.832 _i- 53933.6 H" . ouuaom H on» ~hucnnouunwa xuepvoom Aneuawvsn Hocuouxm - .ooeoou\«eouv \udom —1uoncom nouuuuoaoo xuonvooh - anaconda \uuouoauoucu w EV a «m awfiuwfi 3888.... sequence: N uoueuuoeou oouuquun ucoamuan ooueunuuu< xuoaeoou I111 avenues» oooeauouuom sooaouu unsounan euoeauouuom noeuoooou voyeuocoo \ounuoououou headcouuoan voucuocuu anouueuxu enwoeeuux suenvoom Naomi 63368 .— uououooloo noun—owed r A. . shuuonouuuwa _ sueavoom owcegu no» Hecuuuxm gauuenou .. nun-nuance \uaom «new I11 _ oz 2.... - I“-.. aeou I. u cued owoeauaeoo I 108 discussed in detail below. First, feedback sign and feedback discrepancy should be regarded as two distinct ways of comparing self- and other-generated feedback. They did not interact in influencing external feedback acceptance, but each had a unique effect on this dependent variable. In control theory terms, each can be thought of as a comparator, or a dimension of comparison between self- and external feedback. Feedback discrepancy and feedback sign independently influenced the decision to accept or discount feedback from an external source. As such, each uniquely influenced external feedback acceptance. These results are most likely due to the unique conceptualization of feedback sign. Since sign did not necessarily reflect an evaluation of being above or below standard, it did not interact with feedback discrepancy as predicted. Future research should investigate the influence of traditional feedback sign and feedback discrepancy on feedback acceptance. Second, neither self-confidence nor source credibility interacted with 9913 feedback sign and feedback discrepancy to influence external feedback acceptance. Source credibility did not interacted with either of these variables and self-confidence only interacted with feedback discrepancy. Restriction of range occurred for both of these variables and the self-confidence variable was developed post-hoc. Therefore, the model has not been modified to directly reflect these results. 109 A change has been made to indicate the direct influence of self- confidence and source credibility on external feedback acceptance. The moderating effects of self-confidence and source credrhility on the relationship between feedback discrepancy and external feedback acceptance and feedback sign and external feedback acceptance remain in the model. The problems which occurred in this study, in measuring source credibility and self-confidence may have significantly influenced the results. Changes made in the model related to these variables should therefore be considered tentative, and in need of replication. Note that the variable of self-confidence replaced self-certainty in the revised model. In retrospect, self-confidence appears to reflect the faith one has in self-ratings and in one’s own ability to rate performance. Self-confidence can be thought of as a variable that parallels source credibility. Source credibility indicates the feedback recipient’s faith in and perceived ability of, the external source of feedback. Therefore, self-confidence should moderate the relationship between self-feedback and feedback acceptance just as source credibility should moderate the relationship between external feedback and feedback acceptance. In an attempt to better understand the influence of self— confidence and source credibility in the model, post-hoe hypotheses were proposed regarding these variables. First, the possibility that self-confidence moderated the 110 relationship between self-feedback and consensus performance judgment was tested. This hypothesis suggested that self-feedback would influence the final judgment made regarding performance more for those high in self-confidence, than for those low in self-confidence. Similarly, the possibility that source credibility moderated the relationship between external feedback and consensus performance judgment was tested. This hypothesis suggested that the external feedback would influence the final judgment made regarding performance more when source credibility was high, than when source credibility was low. No support was found for either of these suppositions. Self-feedback was found to directly influence consensus performance judgment and as hypothesized, external feedback was moderated by external feedback acceptance in influencing consensus performance judgment. The final change made in the model was the elimination of the variable of self-feedback acceptance. If one has faith in the performance ratings he/she has provided, as reflected by self-confidence, self feedback acceptance becomes a moot issue. Therefore, this variable does not appear in the revised model of self-other feedback congruence. Research Implications Results of the present study demonstrate the importance of feedback discrepancy for acceptance of external feedback. Note however that feedback acceptance is not an end in and of itself. According to llgen et al. (1979) 111 feedback acceptance acts as a mediator in the relationship between the provision of feedback and the intention to act based on the feedback. The present research can be extended by investigating the influence of feedback discrepancy on the motivation to act based on the feedback. In addition, the relationship between feedback discrepancy and performance deserves further attention. Barley (1988) has found that feedback acceptance is related to performance improvements and has demonstrated the importance of self-generated feedback. No research has yet looked at the effects of feedback discrepancy on performance. Due to the influence of feedback discrepancy on feedback acceptance and the importance placed on self-feedback, feedback discrepancy should be negatively related to motivation to improve and to actual performance improvements. Feedback discrepancy was manipulated in the present research. Future research should be conducted in real work settings to determine the extent to which discrepancies actually occur between self-feedback and feedback generated by an external source. A review by Fisher and Rusk (1987), proposed that self-superior disagreement occur relatively frequently. They also reviewed literature which provide explanations for these disagreements. Future research should utilize these theories in an attempt to identify methods of reducing discrepancies between self- and superior assessments of performance. Such research should avoid the assumption that feedback provided by a 112 superior is "correct". Both self- and superior assessments should be compared to more objective sources of feedback (e.g. task feedback) to determine their respective validities. Previous research related to self-assessments in organizational settings has focused on outcome issues such as the validity or accuracy of self-assessments. Some researchers report that individuals are not skilled at provided self- assessments which are accurate (e.g. Levine, Flory & Ash, 1977) while other researchers suggest individuals have limited capabilities for accurate self- assessments (Mabe & West, 1982; Shrauger & Osberg, 1981). Results of the present research indicate that self-assessments are key components to overall evaluations people make regarding their performance. Results also reveal that differences between self- and other assessments influence acceptance of external feedback. Therefore, future research is needed which investigates methods of making self-assessments more accurate. One way of enhancing the accuracy of self-assessments is by ensuring a similar frame of reference with regard to the standards of performance. Taylor et a1. (1984) suggest that superiors and subordinates should have a clear understanding of the criteria for effective performance. They should also agree that these criteria are valid. This can be accomplished through encouraging participative communication between the two parties (O’Reilly & Anderson, 1980). In addition, feedback should be provided by a credible source. Taylor 113 et al. (1984) further recommend frequent feedback sessions which would increase the perceived accuracy of performance feedback or feedback acceptance. This study supports the view that individuals are more willing to accept feedback which is consistent with their self-view than feedback which is inconsistent with self-perceptions (Shrauger, 1975; Swarm & Read, 1981, Swarm & Hill, 1982). From an organizational perspective, these findings imply that any incongruity between the feedback provider’s and feedback recipient’s perception of the adequacy of performance would be detrimental to acceptance of external feedback and should be reduced. Finally, the results of this study have important implications for the performance feedback process which takes place in organizations. First, it suggests that self-processes, specifically self-feedback, influence individual reactions to performance feedback provided by others. This finding is consistent with models of self-regulation. The integration of the control theory and self-management perspectives suggests that individuals attempt to regulate behavior in relation to a standard based on feedback from multiple sources (e.g. self, other). The control theoretic perspective suggests that discrepancies between perceived environmental feedback and standards (in this case self-feedback) are regulated (Powers, 1973). Previous studies utilizing control theory as a framework have 114 not addressed the possibility that individuals provide self-feedback, however control theory can be applied in these situations. Self-management theorists recognize the fact that individuals observe and evaluate their own behavior (Bandura, 1977, 1978; Kanfer & Karoly, 1982). They further believe that individual attempt to regulate their behavior in relation to a standard. Our findings indicate that selflevaluations of performance strongly influence judgments made regarding the adequacy of performance. These results suggest that self-feedback can serve as a standard against which feedback from other sources in compared. Limitations ‘ The limitations of this study deserve mention. First, due to an error in assignment of participants to experimental conditions, the design was not completely randomized. Data was collected in six out of ten conditions, after which, data was collected in the remaining four conditions. An analysis of individual differences between the two groups was completed and revealed no significant differences between them. Any differences which may have occurred in experimenter behavior from early to late in the study were indeterminable. Participants who were assigned to the final four experimental conditions received instructions from experimenters who over time became more experienced in the experimental procedure. 115 An issue which is often advanced in discussing experiments conducted in laboratory settings in the generalizability of findings. Clearly this study lacks what has been termed "mundane realism" which is the degree to which the experimental setting matches things which occur in organizations (Berkowitz & Donnerstein, 1982). No study is completely generalizable (Fromkin & Struefert, 1976) but the strength of laboratory investigation lies in it’s ability to control irrelevant variables and test causal hypotheses. llgen (1986) points out that high fidelity (physical similarity between laboratory and field settings) can increase the generalizability of laboratory findings. However, he further suggests that if laboratory subjects attribute the same meaning to the variables of interest as subjects in organizations would, high "psychological" fidelity is achieved. The purpose of this study was to determine how individuals react to performance feedback. The process individuals undertake in determining the acceptability of feedback was of primary interest. Therefore, high psychological fidelity was considered more important than high physical fidelity. One issue related to the generalizability of the findings is that the feedback recipient typically has an ongoing relationship with the feedback provider in real work settings. Since there was no one actually providing feedback in this study, the influence of this type of relationship was not assessed. Research conducted in an organizational setting could ascertain the 116 influence of the quality and length of the feedback recipient/feedback provider relationship on reactions to performance feedback. The lack of experience subjects had with the task they were asked to complete also limits the generalizability of this study. The task was chosen to maximize variance on self-certainty. Future research should assess self- assessments of performance on familiar tasks to determine if they influence consensus performance judgments more than that found in the present study. Appendices 117 Appendix A IN-BASKET EXERCISE Your name is 1&9 Your work for the Bennett Corporation. You will be transferred in one week to replace John Quitt who left suddenly. Your employer, the Bennett Corporation, has decided to send you to the Midwest office to go through John Quitt’s (your predecessor’s) in-basket. It is Sunday evening April 13th, and no one else is in the office. In 45 minutes you must catch a plane to a training center where you will be for one week and cannot be contacted. Read through the information, memos and reports. Respond to each item in writing. Responses to these items may include letter writing, memos to others or to your self and scheduling meetings. You may write your response on the same memo you received or no the memo pad provided writing paper is also provided for letter writing. §e_s_urp to attach any letters or memos to the appropriate item. An organizational chart is provided for your reference. You have 20 minutes to go through your in-basket. IT IS ADVISABLE TO READ THROUGH THE ENTIRE IN-BASKET BEFORE TAKING ANY ACTION. 1 18 BENNETT CORPORATION Organization Chart for Midwest Branch Fred Waters Branch Manager Betsy Ferris Secretarial Tom Smith Zone Sales Manager Carla Dwyer Manager,Computer Install/Repair Lee Stanley Dave Thompson Sales Repair Tina Merloff Cathy French Sales Repair Susan Krane Vince Turen Sales Repair Dan Larson Sales Roy Turner Sales Mary Shop Sales Phyllis Whipple Secretarial 119 Approximate Time Required for Various Types of Sales Calls Initial Sales Call ime Regular calculators 2 hrs. Programmable calculators 2 hrs. Computer system 4 hrs. Demonstration Regular calculators 2 hrs. Programmable calculators 3 hrs. Computer systems B10—B20 4 hrs. B40-B55 6 hrs. B70-B90 8 hrs. Regular Calculators Deluxe Calculator Regular Programmable Calculator Deluxe Programmable Calculator Programs - pre-packaged Programs - tailor made Computers Mini Computers Medium Size Computers Large Computers 120 Brim Model No. B2 B3 X-R.5 X-R.7 Mn B10 B12 B15 B19 B40 B44 B48 B50 B55 B70 B74 B75 B77 B80 B84 B88 B90 .Cfit. 200.00 290.00 1400.00 1850.00 125.00 200.00 Basic Cost 26,000 28,000 36,000 41,000 75,000 80,000 97,000 102,000 110,000 375,000 390,000 450,000 470,000 725,000 810,000 915,000 1,325,000 121 3......“ Corporation 3 a1; ' V7 ' 14¢» a: ,4/7g Jami. (067:4 we...” my .1 flux...- «A; M. 'Jm mafia“; ./»C.. [/47 «A W 7»wa J8 M J M A eye” {A W7 71% %/ M3 era-£6. fly...) .1 My A»: ‘65qu 136%)”: _ ' fly Eon... T. SHITH 122 5.6 582533 .wreomcu 92¢ 25555». 20 520 LIPIDQZUO «I.» wdaouowuocn when. 3.3.25 203. 9330 Yet itcrropwoé we: 3.... downs. wczuiJEmaH :66 516C $1.3 . 43323.9 rezoned 2,5 885.95.26 2 8a szo 288: Saw .E IE... r92: ‘15: .5333: .qaoyEot or... 0:8: 232.2282 3:. $5.5: .5 79...? 35576.0 «2. 825:3 cash £32 «3313 ore v. 35;. $93 1:37 7 In: .3: 71C .0; $0579 02 Us: 3;: 32.18 #6650 {2: c {at 20 >36: 99% Isa: .8125 E. 28:: E E: 2:8 0:5 no 58 So 3... adage 32:33 watt. . 23333.3 w _. 553 33433 .C «3.3 9:983:13 31¢ I: .707 35.3. v83 .7300 3,3ro nECSDUuto 4.520%. 57.34: use 3.20 303: #5239: So .63: 526 or... aoeutau 4.855 33. ulna $272123: 2... 936595 35.8 £25619 02 r. 2.6 one 283.52. ID“- Q t LEHSQMW. 0383 he“... batfibl- . ”guitaciwountnx “SEQ MCI IS 23...}: 3.: - s .- mcwuaflbowfflwn do”? J..C.S.d 33¢ foe... Cuban I .239 501965 who... .133 .t. .392 . a 2.2 .Q 33 5.13 Democracy mu 731655239 5.53 .30 >59 bro 2.... 1525.. 98. .56 E .35: we.“ 524:3 63:33:62 out accelcexw .5360 fl 2300 55¢ a. at 22:. 29:5: .076. c «05303. Eu 0 H >3 9:039? .flpucwasn C .6306 use 2.5.; we 9033.5... 9 :89. or... 1.29% . E 39.. out“ an .43an 45:33. you? a mH zeiaou .wt Satousvo was. on :32: >53. 95323: .flwcameu 20 SEEMn .336 .33 . «no-» .3 Um... 330m .wuzaoiou 8 «atomic USDEEO r55 1. on 01 €th 621.595 323 u: u.» 6.6:. "Zen {:1 .266 EECV 356.. 0N9 5:; 9.3.. 95553332 ~30 . 1.5.9532” yearn 250 {2,513 533 22.3533 032.. .9 n 3.... Ed 3332: .ZchcuuoEvto .mcDrCIueioU 33:3 .8 I388 hereunto cur.) 33cm «H 3233 «SE d .230 2.: 05125 Cauck (50 w..41:3.3e. .25.E~u2¢sco 4:233:25 >50 EEDJSFEEV .mp6 9. 2.5.... 4.50 I CU? p3 (ecu 77C 8 craves: L02 $33.53: or: 8.384 Jon—L233... re: :6 «32.555. E3523 5530.5 .353 3.52 951539: ST: _ :92... yeah 3:: .Jofiiruu 1.552 4 .9020 .56.. bwzaufiowwc .2 «2H: EH3 .303 (Zoe an: 20 53:38.03 5622306 55¢ . or... 2 2F E: 3 3:5 .tyzfiuo 3 tree .3: . Binnie .55 «one: 3218 .39: n :23: «£3 22 NWEPrSJ . 23 2.6a. am. . 76510 Eur. 3b .3 “5).—I .5 AL. U E Iver: #03556 C 123 Defunct Corpornt 10!: Q ‘. we. Waéa’mwmfi . agar/W M ,4... .IWMWK'IO- m 124 ”til .11 .12 .73 .w 44" .100 3.0513.“- Tucson ummrsnm' Tawnsmv rmon' M 30:00 IlcOU “00 1100 3:00 , 0:00 SIOO 5100 125 Klingon lndustries lobk Spock Street Iesteen. Kl Harch 21 lennett Corporation 1555 15th Street lesteen, Hl hr. Quitt: Regarding our conversation of Harch 20. l have been considering your offer to demonstrate the 390 and would like to set up an appointment. 1‘9111 be at a conference for the next tun weeks, but as soon as 1 return 1 would like to see the demonstration. l have, therefore. set aside the norning of April 21 for the denonstration. I am looking forward to seeing you at that ticee Yours truly. . Kathy Cornan Vice President 126 ‘AV. ne‘eéa/m c. any 0’2" m (to, *ldé 7’“ #011 tau/J 5 It! A”: ‘4‘“) M JWJKLJMM F T? - a»... Q seeeo-o-terree 3 Infamy-H 127 um mmnos certs-onto: coeecsenuoeuc oooaea ”.0 . ]eotauo- osev. . . Don t CPL! April 9 . .O’I. O b.‘.'0.. ns Horrison, Plant 89erintendent Stratton Assembly Plant .81. C.C- I80 Printer haliuoctions A ' . ° berial embers ”-5753 through‘ 114755 Us have received reports of proble-s vith the line printer (or the I00. a checking a realised one o! our our technicians has been enabling the printer in an incorrect manner. which results in continuous oaliunc- tioning. The technician was responsible for asso-bling the printers with the above Dentioned serial embers. II you have any reports of problem with these printers. please replace than ad return the eali’u-etionieg printer to es. 128 _ bennett Corporation 1535 13th St Pesteen. on d 75.0, uster— .bate zym /§ ‘57.," J137-FI‘M J”“r Address fitting 1”! I ”r" M— ‘ Signature 951 M Description Unit Price 1”“ ’ 3'75 Cmr’"" ' (370/12 (3%” Sub Total {.220 ill) Sales Tax lZflD Total I Y0? [2W :Au‘érised Signature; I _ Renal/UT (Title) 129 Appendix B MEASURES COMPLETED INFORMED CONSENT DECLARATION The purpose of this project is to examine the determinants of sales performance. In order to comply with professional standards as well as those established by Michigan State University, it is essential that you be made aware of the nature of this research and the rights and responsibilities incurred by both you and the researcher. The nature of this projects involves the investigation of what qualities a good salesperson has and what behaviors lead to effective selling. You will be asked to complete an in-basket exercise and will be provided with an evaluation of your performance. You will also be asked to evaluate your own performance. Finally, you will be asked to react to the evaluation you have given yourself as well as the evaluation which will be provided. The study will take approximately 1.5 hours of time for which you will receive course credit. Please note that the information you supply will be kept completely confidential. The principal investigator, MaryBeth DeGregorio will assume complete responsxbility for maintaining the anonymity of all you responses. . It should be stressed that you can decline to participate or terminate your participation in this project at any time without recrimination. In return for your participation, you will be given course credit as outlined by the Psychology Department Human Subjects Committee and your instructor. If you wish to receive this credit, but do not wish to participate in this study, or decide to terminate your participation prior to the study’s completion, an alternative activity requiring similar commitments of time and effort is available. At the end of the study, a more complete written description of this investigation and its findings will be available from the researcher. Along with the rights listed above, you also incur certain responsibilities. Primarily among these responsibilities is that you must provide information that is, as far as possible, accurate and complete. We also ask that you DO NOT disclose any information related to this project to any other persons until this project is completed (e.g., until the end of Spring Quarter, 1989). I certify that I have read and understand the rights and responsibilities incurred by both me and the researchers in this project as outlined above. (Signature) (Print Name) (Date) 130 Appendix B (cont) Introduction The purpose of this study is to identify what qualities a good salesperson has and what behaviors lead to effective selling. In this study you will be asked to complete an in-basket exercise. An in-basket exercise is a frequently used technique for evaluating sales ability. The in-basket itself, is a collection or group of materials which might be found in the incoming mail of a supervisor or manager who has just taken over the position. A secretary is not available, phones are disconnected and files are locked. Your task is to determine what to do with the various situations presented in the in-basket materials. Please keep in mind that it is not important whether or not you have previous experience in sales. We are interested in the raw talent people possess to be salespersons. It is therefore very important that you try to do your very best at the in-basket exercise. Attached you will find a description of skills that the in-basket exercise measures. Preliminary data indicates that the average in-basket exercise score received by undergraduate students working on an in-basket for the first time is a 50 on a 100 point scale. We would like you to attempt to receive a score of 70. This is a difficult but attainable goal for students with no in-basket exercise experience. 131 Appendix B (cont) IN-BASKET PERFORMANCE Organization and Planning - The participant should develop a plan of attack. Priorities should be set. Time must be used efficiently. Delegation should be used. Conflicts should be recognized and dealt with. Decision Making - Decisions should be made quickly. Procrastination should not take place. The decision should be of high quality given the available information. Problem Analysis - Interrelationships between items should be noted and handled. Additional information should be sought. Customer significance should be noted. Missing information must be recognized and sought out. social Skills - Memos, letters, etc. should fit the situation. Consideration should be demonstrated by the use of thank-you’s and polite language where appropriate. Firmness should be conveyed in writing where appropriate. Unsatis- Average Outstanding factory O 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 132 Appendix B (cont.) In-Basket Performance Evaluation Form (Self Rating) Please provide an OVERALL rating of performance. Your goal for performance was a score of 70. You should CONSIDER the performance dimensions listed below in making ONE overall rating. Please respond by placing any number from 0 to 100, which best describes your performance, in the space provided. Use the 83$ below for assistance in making your rating. 1. Dimension 1 - Organization and Planning A plan of attack was deve10ped. Conflicts were recognized and dealt with. 2. Dimension 2 - Decision Making Decisions were made quickly. Decisions were of high quality given the available information. 3. Dimension 3 - Problem Analysis Interrelationships between items were noted and handled. Missing information was recognized and sought out. 4. Dimension 4 - Social Skills Memos, letters fit the situation. Consideration was demonstrated. Firmness and tact was used when appropriate. Unsatis- Average Outstanding factory 0 10 20 30 4O 50 60 70 8O 90 100 Overall Rating 133 Appendix B (cont.) Self Feedback Acceptance Please consider the ratings that you provided for yourself. Answer the following questions using the scale provided below. Write your responses in the space provided at the left. Strongly Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1) My self feedback did not truly depict my performance on the sales task. 2) My self evaluation reflected my true performance. 3) My self feedback was an accurate evaluation of my performance on the task. 134 Appendix B (cont.) Self Certainty The following questions address how confident you are in the ratings you have just provided. Please use the following scale to answer the questions below. Fill in your response in the space provided at the left. Strongly Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1) I am confident about the self evaluation I provided. 2) I am not sure how I did on the sales task. 3) I am certain that my self-ratings reflect my true performance. 4) I have faith in my self-ratings of performance. 5) I doubt the self-ratings I gave. 135 Appendix B (cont.) In-Basket Performance Evaluation Form (Evaluator Rating) Please provide an OVERALL rating of performance. You should CONSIDER the performance dimensions listed below in making ONE overall rating. Please respond by placing any number from O to 100, which best describes the student’s performance, in the space provided. Use the scale below for assistance in making your rating. 1. Dimension 1 - Organization and Planning A plan of attack was developed. Conflicts were recognized and dealt with. 2. Dimension 2 - Decision Making Decisions were made quickly. Decisions were of high quality given the available information. 3. Dimension 3 - Problem Analysis Interrelationships between items were noted and handled. Missing information was recognized and sought out. 4. Dimension 4 - Social Skills Memos, letters fit the situation. Consideration was demonstrated. Firmness and tact was used when appropriate. Unsatis- Average Outstanding factory . 0 10 20 30 4O 50 60 7O 8O 90 100 Overall Ratin 136 Appendix B (cont.) Shown below are a number of statements concerning your attitudes regarding various topics. Carefully consider each of the following statements with respect to your personal opinions. Then, decide the extent to which each statement describes you. Use the response options shown below. Write the number of your response to the left of each statement. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Always Almost Usually Sometimes Seldom Almost Never Always Never 1. I do my best work when my job assignments are fairly difficult. 2. When I have a choice, I try to work in a group instead of by myself. 3. In my work assignments, I try to be my own boss. 4. I seek an active role in the leadership of a group. 5. I try very hard to improve on my past performance. 6. I pay a good deal of attention to the feelings of others I work with. 7. I go my own way when I work, regardless of the opinions of others. 8. I avoid trying to influence those around me to see things my way. 9. I take moderate risks and stick my neck out to get ahead in my work. 10. I prefer to do my own work and let others do theirs. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 137 I disregard rules and regulations that hamper my personal freedom. I find myself organizing and directing the activities of others. I try to avoid any added responsibilities when I work. I express my disagreements with others openly. I consider myself a "team player" when I work. I strive to gain more control over the events around me. I try to perform better than my fellow students. I find myself talking to those around me about non-school matters. I try my best to work alone on a job. I strive to be "in command" when I am working in a group. 138 Shown below are several more statements concerning your attitudes. Carefully consider each of the following statements and decide the degree to which you agree or disagree with each statement. Use the response options shown below. Write the number of your response to the left of each statement. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Moderately Slightly Neither Slightly Moderately Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Disagree Agree Agree Agree Nor Agree _1. I feel that I’m a person of worth, at least on an equal basis with others. __2. I feel that I have a number of good qualities. _3. All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure. __4. I am able to do things as well as most other people. _5. I feel I do not have much to be proud of. __6. I take a positive attitude toward myself. __7. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself. __8. I wish I could have more respect for myself. _9. I certainly feel useless at times. 10. At times I think I am no good at all. 139 Consider each of the following statements. Choose the statement in each pair of statements that you feel is closest to your opinion. Write the letter indicating your response to the left of each pair of statements. THEN, check the box which best describes how much closer the statement you choose is to your true opinion than the statement you did not choose. Statement closer to my opinion (a or b?) a)Children get into trouble because their parents punish them too much. b)The trouble with most children nowadays is their parents are too easy. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)In the long run people get the respect they deserve in the world. b)Unfortunately, an individual’s worth often passes unrecognized no matter how hard he/she tries. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)The idea that teachers are unfair to students is nonsense. b)Most students don’t realize the extent to which their grades are influenced by accidental happenings. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)Becoming a success is a matter of hard work, luck has little to do with it. b)Getting a good job depends on being in the right place at the right time. Slightly Closer Much Closer 140 a)The average citizen can have an influence in government decisions. b)This world is run by the few people in power, and there is not much the little guy can do about it. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)In my case getting what I want has little or nothing to do with luck. b)Many times we might just as well decide what to do by flipping a coin. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)Who gets to be the boss often depends on who was lucky enough to be in the right place first. b)Getting people to do the right thing depends upon ability, luck has little to do with it. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)Most people don’t realize the extent to which their lives are controlled by accidental happenings. b)There really is no such thing as "luck". Slightly Closer Much Closer a)In the long run the bad things that happen to us are balanced by the good ones. b)Most misfortunes are the result of lack of ability, ignorance, laziness or all of these. Slightly Closer Much Closer 10. 11. 141 a)Many times I feel that I have little influence over the things that happen to me. b)It is impossible for me to believe that chance or luck plays an important role in my life. Slightly Closer Much Closer a)What happens to me is my own doing. b)Sometimes I feel that I don’t have enough control over the direction my life is taking. Slightly Closer Much Closer 142 Appendix B (cont.) Perceived Congruence Please consider the ratings that your evaluator gave you. Answer the following questions using the scale provided below. Write your responses in the space provided at the left. Strongly Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 The evaluation I received was similar to my self evaluation. The rater’s evaluation was inconsistent with my self rating. My self ratings agreed with the ratings I received from the evaluator. My self assessment and the evaluation I received from the rater matched. There was a close correspondence between the rating I received from the evaluator and my own self rating. 143 Appendix B (cont.) Perceived Credibility Items Please consider the ratings that your evaluator gave you. Answer the following questions using the scale provided below. Write your responses in the space provided at the left. Strongly Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 The rater who evaluated my performance was knowledgeable. The rater was incompetent. The rater could be considered an expert evaluator. The rater was skilled in the evaluation of in-basket exercise performance. The rater was inexperienced in performance assessment. 144 Appendix B (cont.) External Feedback Acceptance Items Please consider the ratings that your evaluator gave you. Answer the following questions using the scale provided below. Write your responses in the space provided at the left. Strongly Neither Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 1 ” 2 3 4 5 6 7 The feedback did not truly depict my performance on the sales task. The rater’s evaluation reflected my true performance. The feedback the rater gave me was an accurate evaluation of my performance on the task. I do not feel the feedback reflected my true performance. 145 Appendix B (cont.) Self Rating 11 You will soon be asked to complete a second in-basket exercise. Prior to doing so we ask that you again think about your performance on the previous in-basket exercise. We would like you to CAREFULLY consider your self evaluation of performance as well as the rater’s evaluation of your performance. Please provide an OVERALL rating of performance. Your goal for performance was a score of 70. You should CONSIDER the performance dimensions listed below in making ONE overall rating. Please respond by placing any number from 0 to 100, which best describes your performance, in the space provided. Use the scale below for assistance in making your rating. 1. Dimension 1 - Organization and Planning " A plan of attack was developed. Conflicts were recognized and dealt with. 2. Dimension 2 - Decision Making Decisions were made quickly. Decisions were of high quality given the available information. 3. Dimension 3 - Problem Analysis Interrelationships between items were noted and handled. Missing information was recognized and sought out. 4. Dimension 4 - Social Skills Memos, letters fit the situation. Consideration was demonstrated. Firmness and tact was used when appropriate. Unsatis- Average Outstanding factory 0 10 20 3O 40 50 6O 7O 80 90 100 Overall Rating 146 Appendix B (cont.) DeBriefing This study investigated reactions to performance feedback. You received feedback which was either very similar to your own self-evaluation or dissimilar from your self-evaluation. Half of those who received feedback which was dissimilar from their own self-evaluation received feedback indicating that their performance was poor, the other half received feedback indicating that their performance was good. In addition, you were led to believe that the individual who provided the feedback had either a great deal of expertise (industrial/organizational graduate student) or no expertise in evaluating sales performance (undergraduate student). In fact, the feedback you received was predetermined. We expect that both similarity of other assessments to self assessments and the expertise of the rater will influence feedback acceptance. It should be stressed that the performance feedback you received IN NO WAY reflects your true performance. Your performance was in NO WAY being evaluated. You were randomly assigned to either a group who received feedback similar to your self-view or feedback which was dissimilar from your self-view. Thank you for your time. 147 Appendix C LOW CREDIBILITY (Experimenter Script) Now that you have completed your self-evaluation I am going to take your in-basket to be evaluated by another person. Your performance will be evaluated by a Michigan State University Freshman psychology student who is also participating in this study for course credit. The student has NO previous experience in evaluating performance. The student has no familiarity with in- basket exercises either. In fact, no exposure to in-basket exercises will be provided to this evaluator until the student is asked to assess your performance. The freshman evaluator will be assessing your performance based on the same dimensions you used for self-evaluation. While your performance is being evaluated, please fill out this attitude questionnaire. HIGH CREDIBILITY (Experimenter Script) . Now that you have completed your self-evaluation I am going to take your in-basket to be evaluated by another person. Your performance will be evaluated by the Industrial/Organizational psychology graduate student who is conducting this study to complete requirements for the degree of PhD. This graduate student has worked as a manager for five years prior to entering graduate school, therefore has a great deal of experience in evaluating performance. The graduate student has also received extensive training in the measurement of in-basket exercise performance. In-basket exercises were frequently used at the organization where the student worked. This evaluator now conducts seminars on the use and evaluation of in-basket exercises. The graduate, student evaluator will be assessing your performance based on the same dimensions you used for self-evaluation. While your performance is being evaluated, please fill out this attitude questionnaire. 148 Appendix D Results of factor analysis of self certainty and self feedback acceptance scales. Item Factor 1: Positively Eigenvalue= 4.11 Worded Items Factor Loadings l .2. My self feedback did not truly depict my performance on the sales task. .655 .167 I am confident about the self evaluation I provided. .649 .357 My self evaluation reflected my true performance. .809 .091 I am certain that my self-ratings reflect my true performance. .839 .181 My self feedback was an accurate evaluation of my performance on the task. .817 .163 I have faith in my self-ratings of performance. .618 .569 Factor 2: Negatively Eigenvalue= 1.08 Worded Items I am not sure how I did on the sales task. -.025 .849 I doubt the self-ratings I gave. .395 .717 LIST OF REFERENCES 149 LIST OF REFERENCES Ashford, SJ. & Cummings,LL.(1983).Feedback as an individual resource: Personal strategies of creating information. Organizational Behavior prld Human Performancje, 3;, 370-398. Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. finefican Psychologist, 33, 344-357. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review. 84, 191-215. Bannister, B.D. (1987). Performance outcome feedback and attn’butional feedback: Interactive effects on recipient responses. Journal 91' Applied Psychology, 11, 203-210. Baumeister, RF. (1982). A self-presentational view of social phenomena. Psycholgical Bulletin 21, 3-26. Becker, LJ. (1978). Joint effect of feedback and goal-setting on performance: A field study of residential energy conservation. Journal o_f Applied Psychology, Q, 428-433. Berkowitz, L. & Doonnerstein, E. (1982). External validity is more than skin deep: Some answers to criticisms of laboratory experiments. American Psychologist, 12, 245-237.- Bradley, G.W. (1978). Self-serving biases in the attn’bution process. A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. Journal o_f Personality m Social Psycholcgy, 36, 56-71. Bray, D.W. & Grant, D. (1966). The assessment center in the measurement of potential for business management. Psychological Monoggaphs, 66, 80, (17, Whole No. 625). Brickman, P. (1972). Rational and nonrational elements in reactions to disconfirrnation of performance expectancies. Journal pf E_xpgrimental figia] Bsycholog, 8, 112-123. Campion, M.A. & Lord, R.G. (1982). A control systems conceptualization of the goal-setting and changing process. Organizational Behavior am Human Eefionpapce, 10, 265-288. 150 Carver, C.S. & Scheier, M.F. (1981). Attention apt! Self-regplation: A Control flfheog Approach 39 Human Behayior. New York: Springer-Verlag. Cedarblom, D. (1982). The performance appraisal interview: A review, implications and suggestions. Academy 91' Management Review 1, 219-227. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis M _t_h_e behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cooley, CH. (1902). Human Nature £138 13 Social Order. New York: Scribner. Crary, W.G. (1966). Reactions to incongruent self-experiences. Journal pf Consulting chhology, fl, 246-252. DeNisi, A.S., Randolph, W.A. & Blencoe, AG. (1983). Potential problems with peer ratings. Academy o_f Management Journal, _2_§, 457-464. Dipboye, R.C. (1977). A critical review of Korman’s self-consistency theory of work motivation and occupational Choice. Organizational Behavior m Human Bgrfpmancp, 18, 108-126. Barley, P.C. (1988). Computer-generated performance feedback in the magazine-subscription industry. Organizational Behavior app Human Decision Processes, 4_1, 50-64. Earley, P.C. (1985). Influence of information, choice, and task complexity on goal acceptance, performance, and personal goals. Journal pf Applied Psychology, _7_0_, 481-491. Erez, M. & Zidon, I. (1984). Effect of goal acceptance on the relation of goal difficulty to performance. Journal pf Applied Psychology, 82, 69-78. Erez, M. (1977). Feedback: A necessary condition for the goal-setting performance relationship. Journal pf Applied Psychology, 82, 624-627. Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. uman Relations, 1, 117-140. Fisher, C.D. & Russ, G. (1987). Self and superior assessment. Technical Report for the Office of Naval Research: N00014-85-k-0289: TR-ONR-4. College Station, TX, Department of Management. 151 Giffin, K. (1967). The contribution of studies of source credibility to a theory of interpersonal trust in the communication process. [gchological Bulletin Q, 104-120. Greller, M.M. & Herold, D.M. (1975). Sources of feedback: A preliminary investigation. Organizational Behavior m Human Performance 18, 244-256. Greller, M.M. (1980). Evaluation of feedback sources as a function of role and organizational level. Journal 8f Applied P cholo , Q, 16-23. Hackman, J.R. & Oldham, GR. (1976). Motivation through the design of work. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 18, 250-279. Halperin, K., Snyder, C.R., Shenkel, R.J., & Houston, B.I(. (1976). Effectiof source status and message favorability on acceptance of personality feedback. Journal 8f Applied chhology, 81, 85-88. Hanser, L.M. & Muchinsky, RM. (1978). Work as an information environment. Organizational Behavior apg Human Performance, 81, 47-60. Harris, M.M. & Schaubroeck, J. (1988). A meta-analysis of self- supervisor, self-peer, and peer supervisor ratings. Egrsonnel Psychology, fl, 43-62. Harvey, OJ. & Clapp, W.F. (1965). Hope, expectancy, and reactions to the unexpected. Journal 81’ Eersonality g8 Social Psychology, 2, 45-52. Herold, D.M. & Greller, M.M. (1977). Feedback: The definition of a construct. Academy 91‘ Management Journal, 28, 142-147. Herold, D.M., Liden, R.C. & Leatherwood, MJ... (1987). Using multiple attributes to assess sources of performance feedback. Academy pf Management Journal 89, 826-835. Hillery, J.M. & Wexley, K.N. (1974). Participation effects in appraisal interviews conducted in a training situation. Journal 91' Applied Psychology, 89, 168-171. Hollenbeck, J.R. & Brief, AP. (1988). Self-regulation in the workplace: Towards a unified approach to understanding worker attitudes and behaviors. In R. Schuler (Ed.) Readings i_n Personnel 898 Human Resources Management. St. Paul, MN.: West. 152 Hollenbeck, J. R. & Williams, CR. (1987). Goal importance, self focus and the goal setting process. Journal pf Applied Psychology, 22, 204-211. llgen, DR. (1986). Laboratory research: A question of when, not if. In E.A. Locke (Ed). Generalizing 80m laboratory 19 8818 settings. llgen, D.R., Fisher, C.D. & Taylor, MS. (1979). Consequences of individual feedback on behavior in organizations. Journal _o_f Applied Psychology, fl, 349-371. llgen, D.R. & Moore, CF. (1987). Types and choices of performance feedback. Journal 81' Applied Psychology, 1;, 401-406. llgen, D.R., Peterson, R.B., Martin, B.A. & Boeschen, DA. (1981). Supervisor and subordinate reactions to performance appraisal sessions. Organizational Behavior _a_n_g Human Performance, _2_1, 437-450. Jones, E.E. & Nisbett, RE. (1971). DLe actor 8n_d 818 observer: General Learning Press. Jones, S.C. & Schneider, DJ. (1968). Certainty of self-appraisals and reactions to evaluations from others. Sociometg, ,3_1, 395-403. Kanfer, F.H. & Karoly, P. (1982). The psychology of self-management: Abiding issues and tentative directions. In Paul Karoly & Fredrick H. Kanfer (Eds) 8ng Management m Behavior Qhange Em Theogy _tp Practice. NY: Pergamon Press. Kay, E., Meyer, H.H. & French, J.R.P., Jr. (1965). Effects of threat in a performance appraisal interview. Journal o_f Applied Psychology, Q, 311-317. Klein, HJ. (1987). Reactions to goal setting and feedback: A test of a control theory model of work motivation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Michigan State University. Komaki, J., Barwick, K.D. & Scott, I... R. (1978). A behavioral safety approach to occupational safety: Pinpointing and reinforcing safe performance in a food manufacturing plant. Journal 8f Applied Psychology, 83_, 434-445. 153 Korman, A. (1970). Toward a hypothesis of work behavior. Journal 8f Applied Bgchology, 53, 31-41. Kuiper, N.A. & Rogers, T.B. (1979). Encoding of personal information: Self-other differences. Joumal 8f Bersonalig 888 Social Psychology, fl, 499-514. Larson, J.R., Jr. (1984). The performance feedback process: A preliminary model. Organizational Behavior m Human Performance, 88, 42-76. Lawler, BE. (1967). the multitrait-multirater approach to measuring managerial job performance. Journal 8f Applied Esychology, 81, 369-381. Levine, E.L., Flory, A. & Ash, RA (1977). Self-assessment in personnel selection. Journal 81‘ Applied Esychology, 82, 428-435. Locke, EA. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. _Cflganizational Behavior 888 Human Eerformance, 8, 157-189. Locke, E.A., Shaw, K.N., Saari, L.M., & Latham, GP. (1981). Goal setting and task performance: 1969-1980. Egchologjcal Bulletin, 98, 125-152. Lord, R.G. & Hanges, PJ. (1987). A control system model of organizational motivation: Theoretical development and applied implications. Behavioral fijence, 82, 161-178. Manz, CC. (1986). Self-leadership: Toward an expanded theory of elf-influence processes in organizations. Academy o_f Management Review, 11, 585-600. Marek, J. & Mettee, DR. (1972). Avoidance of continued success as a function of self-esteem, level of esteem certainty and responsibility for success. Journal pf Personalig 8118 Social Psychology, Q, 98-107. Markus, H. (1977). Self schemata and processing information about the self. Journal o_f Eersonalig gig Social Psychology, 3_5_, 63-78. Mead, OH. (1934). Mind, §£Lf 3.119. Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 154 Meyer, W.U. & Starke, E. (1982). Own ability in relation to self-concept of ability: A field study of information seeking. Bersonalig 88d M81 mm fi. 501-507- Miller, D.T. (1976). Ego involvement and attributions for success and failures. Journal pf mm £119 m1 Eachology, 85, 901-906. Miller, G.A., Galanter, E., & Pribaum, K.H. (1960). Blans _a_ng 189 structure pf behavior. New York: Holt, Reinhart, and Winston. O’Reilly, C.A. & Anderson, J. (1980). Trust and the communication of performance appraisal information: The effects of feedback on performance and job satisfaction. Human Communication Research 8, 290-298. Parker, J.W., Taylor, E.K., Barrett, R.S. & Martens, L. (1959). Rating scale content: 111. Relationship between supervisory- and self-ratings. Personnel Egchology, 18, 49-63. Podaskoff, P.M. & Farh, J. (In Press). Effects of feedback sign and credibility on goal setting and task performance. Organizational Behavior m Human Performance 43. Powers, W.T. (1973). Behavior: 1h; Control 8f Perception. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company. Prien, E.P. & Liske, RE. (1962). Assessments of higher level personnel: III. A comparative analysis of supervisor ratings and incumbent self-ratings of job performance. Bersonnel chhology, 18, 187-194. Rogers, T.B., Kuiper, N.A., & Kirker, W.S. (1977). Self reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal o_f Personalig 888 Social Psychology, 88, 677-688. Sachs, P.R. (1982). Avoidance of diagnostic information in self- evaluation of ability. Personality 13ch Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 242-246. Secord, P.F. & Backman, CW. (1961). Personality theory and the problems of stability and change in individual behavior. An interpersonal approach. Psychological Review, 88, 21-32. Shrauger, 1.5. (1975). Responses to evaluation as a function of initial self-perceptions. Esychological Bulletin, 88, 581-196. 155 Shrauger, J.S. & Rosenberg, SE. (1970). Self-esteem and effects of success and failure feedback on performance. Journal pf Eggsonality, 88, 404-417. Shrauger, J.S. & Schoeneman, J. (1979). Symbolic interactionist view of self-concept: Through the looking glass darkly. Rsygllglpglgal Bulletip, 88, 549-5 73. Smith, MB. (1968). The self and cognitive consistency. In R.B. Abelson et al. (Eds). Theories 8f cogpitive consistepgy: A sourcebook. Chicago: Rand McNally. Snyder, M. & Swarm, W.B., Jr. (1978). Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: From social perception to social reality. Journal 91 Emrimental Social Egghglggy, 11, 148-162. Stone, D.L., Guetal, H.G. & McIntosh, B. (1984). The effects of feedback sequence and expertise of the rater on perceived feedback accuracy. Bersonnel chhology, L7. 487-506. Stone, D.L. & Stone, ER (1985). The effects of feedback consistency and feedback favorability on self-perceived task competence and perceived feedback accuracy. W Behavip; an_d Hm mcision Brocesses, E, 167-185. Stone, E.F. & Stone, DJ... (1984). The effects of multiple sources of performance feedback favorability on self-perceived task competence and perceived feedback accuracy. Journal 91' Managemeng, 11), 371-378. Strang, H.R., Lawrence, E.C., & Fowler, P.C. (1978). Effects of assigned goal level and knowledge of results on arithmetic computation: A laboratory study. Journal _o_f Applied Psychology, _68, 446-450. Strube, M.J. & Roemmele, LA. (1985). Self-enhancement, self- assessment and self-evaluative task choice. Journal _o_f Personalig a_ng Social Psychology, 89, 981-993. Swann, W.B. (1982). Self-Verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A.G. Greenwald (Eds) Psychological Perspectives pp 188 ,S_el_f, 8, 33-66. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 156 Swann, W.B. & Hill, CA. (1982). When our identities are mistaken: Reaffirming self-conceptions through social interaction. Journal _o_f Eersonalig g8 Social chhology, 48, 59-66. Swann, W.B. & Read, SJ. (1982). Self-verification processes: How we sustain our self-conceptions. Journal pf flpgrimental Social chhology, 11, 351-372. Swarm, W.B. & Read, SJ. (1981). Acquiring self-knowledge: The search for feedback that fits. Journal 91' Personalig an_d _S8c1_'8l chhology, 4_1, 1119-1128. Taylor, S.M., Fisher, C.D. & llgen, DR. (1984). Individuals’ reactions to performance feedback in organizations: A control theory perspective. In K. M. Rowland & G. R. Ferris (Eds), Research in Bersonnel a_n_d_ Human Resources Management, 8, 81-124. Greenwich, Conn: JAI Press. Thornton, GO (1968). The relationship between supervisory and self-appraisals of executive performance. Bersonnel chhology, a. 441-455. Thornton, G.C. III. (1980). Psychometric properties of self-appraisals of job performance. Bersonnel Esychology, 88, 263-272. animal and 81; machine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. l'. L) “la :02 ‘1 -'-flm 111041an STATE UNIV. LIBRnRIEs mW"Wll‘l‘lilllHllllllllllUlllWWIIIWI'HI 31293007591658