'{€“:‘:'1J" '4." m~ .>...H‘.. :4.“ ’5‘ - I”... My...“ ._ ”V’YH: ’ I ..‘,-', llllllllllllllllllllHlllIllllllllllllHlllllllHlllllUlllll L 31293 00779 5416 j A r T ‘1 LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Toward a Definition of Aliteracy among Community College Students presented by Raelyn Agustin Joyce has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. English degree in WM 4/ KM Major professor Date 30 ”744/! /7&// , \j 7 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 m ... 1/“ ‘ " . I ..’ f i/ ‘l ' . V ’ \ ‘ ”>5 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE DEC 9 0 1995 MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution c:\clrc\datndue.pm3—p.1 TOWARD A DEFINITION OF ALITERACY AMONG COMMUNITY COLLEGE STUDENTS BY Raelyn Agustin Joyce A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of English 1991 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII------—-———————————‘ ~1‘7’/5 675%“ ABSTRACT TOWARD A DEFINITION OF ALITERACY AMONG COMMUNITY COLLEGE STUDENTS BY Raelyn Agustin Joyce A survey of the literature on aliteracy demonstrates the need to define the term precisely. Much of the litera- ture laments the "problem" of aliteracy without explaining what it is and without citing evidence that it exists. The aims of this study were to define and explicate aliteracy; to determine the accuracy of claims that aliteracy is widespread; to find out to what extent aliteracy exists among community college students; and to explain the impli— cations of alitercy in the light of new, comprehensive definitions of literacy. To explicate aliteracy, I reviewed the literature on literacy, literacy instruction, aliteracy, and topics related to aliteracy (e.g., reading reluctance). Using questionnaires, I conducted a survey of students at Kalama- zoo Valley Community College (KVCC) and interviewed nine students who through their answers showed aliterate tend- encies. Defined as a condition of having skills in reading but not utilizing these skills, aliteracy, like literacy, needs to be viewed as a complex, multi-dimensional, and contextual phenomenon. It represents a pattern of thinking and behaving towards print. Aliteracy is a continuum, involving these components: attitude toward reading, read- ing behavior, types of text read, motivations for reading, intensity of motivations for reading, and reading ability. These components, each a continuum, interact with each other to form a configuration that may be highly individu- al, leading to the conclusion that there are many types of aliteracy. The results of the survey suggest that the KVCC sample is similar to the American adult population: a very high percentage read newspapers, magazines, 9; books, but almost half of the sample did not read books and showed aliterate characteristics (they tended to have a negative attitude toward reading, read less, read fewer types of texts, have fewer motivations for reading, have lower intensity of motivations for reading, and have lower self-perceived reading ability.) A conclusion of the study is that stu— dents who showed aliterate characteristics yieg§g_th§m; §§IX§§ as people who did pg; like to read, especially DQQKfi, and as people who avoided reading. Their negative view of reading and poor perception of themselves as read- ers seemed to have come from school reading experiences. g':;-,.=_ .>.,_(1. ‘ Cc 19 Copyright by RAELYN AGUSTIN JOYCE 1 9 9 1 To my mother, Efigenia Soriano Agustin (1900— ), an untiring reader and writer. Since she learned to read and write (from the first wave of American teachers who went to the Philippines in the start of this century ), her literacy has been her hallmark. She became a teacher after only seven years of formal schooling and received commendations for her exem- plary teaching. She became a mother of eleven children. Her days were arduous as she helped earn a living for her family. But her nights were devoted to Bible—reading, meditation and learning. As her youngest child, I have vivid memories of her reading and writing by kerosene light at all hours of the night, to wake up before we did at dawn to start another busy day. She had a dream that each of her children would re- ceive the education that she didn’t have. On behalf of her children, all of whom have earned college degrees, I thank her for her dream. Without her groundwork, this disserta— tion could never have been written. ""1ch (D ‘1‘ III‘ 1 I1 II- ] . I ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to acknowledge the many people who have con— tributed to the writing of this dissertation: President Marilyn Schlack, Vice President Helen McCauslin, and Dean Betty Chang and others at Kalamazoo Valley Community College for their encouragement and sup— port of my work. The instructors at Kalamazoo Valley Community College who invited me into their classes and allowed me to admin- ister the survey to their students: Pat Baker, Marlin Gerber, Robert Haight, Doug Howe, Nick Meier, Sadie Miles, Julie Moulds, Natalie Patchell, and Dick Phillips. The students at Kalamazoo Valley Community College for participating in the study. I especially want to thank the nine students who, in willingly talking about their lives as readers, helped me understand the nature of aliteracy. Keith Kroll and Ellen Brinkley for the rides, the conversations, the faith, and the fun that we shared. The members of my guidance committee: Professors Stephen Tchudi (chair), Diane Brunner, Sheila Fitzgerald, and Marilyn Wilson-—not just for the helpful comments on the dissertation but also for the support and encouragement they have given me as a doctoral student. My special thanks to Steve Tchudi, whose manifold talents as teacher, scholar, researcher, and editor have inspired me and made possible the speedy completion of this dissertation. His interest and confidence in my work has meant much to me. Finally, my family——my husband Dick and children Rebecca and Andrew for their love and support. Dick has helped in countless ways. His unfailing patience and willingness to help at all times has sustained me through- out the writing of this dissertation. vi LIS'. LIS'. CHA] CHAI CHAI TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER I - INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . General Statement of Purpose . . . . . . . Background and Rationale . . . . . . . ~Rationale for Defining "Aliteracy“ and other Related Terms . . . . . . . . . . . Research Questions . . . . . . . . . . . . Limitations of the Study . . . . . . . . . Organization of the Study . . . . . . . . CHAPTER II - LITERACY AND ITS RELATIONSHIP TO ALITERACY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . raLiteracy: Multiple Meanings . . . . . . . .Promotion of Basic Literacy: Consequences .Literacy and Schooling . . . . . . . . . . ‘CHAPTER III — REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . . ”Literature on Reading Reluctance . . . . . :Literature on Aliteracy . . . . . . . . . . 'Attitudes toward Reading . . . . . . . . . Reading Habits and Readership Surveys . . . The Extent of Aliteracy in Society . . . . Indicators of Aliteracy . . Reading Attitude and Reading Behavior Book Reading . . . . . . . . . . . Range of Texts Read . . . . . . Range and Intensity of Motivations for Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . Reading Ability . . . . . . . . . . . Gender . . . . . . . . Reading in Relation to other Activities . . . . Toward a Comprehensive Definition of Aliteracy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xi P‘H H OKDWU CHAPT] CHAPT CHAP'I CHAPTER IV - METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . Research Questions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 Research Design . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Rationale for Survey and Interviews . Research Methodology Assumptions . . . . . . . 85 Survey . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Selection of Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Pilot study . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Construction of Questionnaire . . . . . . . . . 89 Interviews . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Data Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . 94 Answering the Research Questions . . . . . . 95 CHAPTER V - INTERVIEWS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97 Profiles of Nine Students . . . . . . . . . . . 97 Bob . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97 Lee . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 Tom . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 Sue . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104 Ann . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 Ruth . . . . . . . . Joe . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112 Ron . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 Keith . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 Students’ Perceptions of Themselves as Readers . . . . . . . . . 128 Students’ Views of Book Reading . . . . . 128 School Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 Home Environment . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 Reading Ability . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 Reasons for Reading . . . . . . . . 130 Students’ Attitudes Toward Their Reading Habits . . . . . . . . . . . 131 CHAPTER VI - ANALYSIS OF RESULTS . . . . . . Aliteracy in American Society . . . . . . . 132 Aliteracy among Kalamazoo Valley Community College Students . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 Reading Habits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 Non-Book Reading . . . . . . 137 The Nature of Aliteracy Among KVCC Students . Students’ Perceptions of the Influences on Their Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 144 O 138 Home Environment . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 School Environment . . . . . . . . . . . . 151 Reading Attitudes . . . . . . . . . . . 154 Gender as a Predictor of Aliteracy . . . . . . 156 Reasons for Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159 Types of Texts Read . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 163 Reading Ability Multi-dimensional Nature of Aliteracy . CHAPTER VII - CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Popular Beliefs about Aliteracy versus Findings of the Study Recommendations for Educators and Implications for Classroom Teaching Future Research . . APPENDIX A: APPENDIX B: LIST OF REFERENCES ix 0 The Survey Questionnaire 0 Approval for Human Subjects Research 164 168 173 ' 174 197 211 215 222 223 Talc LIST OF TABLES- Table Page 1 Reasons for Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 2 Positive and Negative Influences on Reading Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147 3 Liking to Read vs. Reading for Enjoyment . . . 155 Figl igure 1 2 10 11 LIST OF FIGURES Comparison of BISG and KVCC Results Attitudes toward Reading . . . . . . Types of Texts Read Variety of Motivations o o o o o o o Intensity of Motivations . . . . . . Self-Perceived Reading Ability . . . Gender and Reading Behavior . . . Gender and Attitude Toward Reading . Overlapping Components of Aliteracy Components of Literacy and Aliteracy . "A Vicious Circle: Nonreaders produce nonreaders" Page 136 139 140 141 142 143 157 158 179 188 203 ex 11 de mi ri CC CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION eral Statement of Purpose In this dissertation, my goal is to clarify and licate the concept of aliteracy through a review of the erature and through a study of community college stu- ts. More specifically, I will develop an extended defi- ion of aliteracy, identifying its indicators and catego- s. I will determine if aliteracy exists among community lege students and, if it does, to what extent. Final- I will discuss the significance and implications of teracy in relation to new, comprehensive definitions of eracy. (ground and Rationale The motivation for this study grew from comments I a received from community college students in children’s arature classes over a period of seven years. Asked to 30nd to a local newspaper editorial contrasting illit- :y and aliteracy (the latter defined as having the ab. ve: tit as re th ha ot of a1 te 3i tlity to read but choosing not to), students have re- iled in journals, in class discussions, or in conversa- >ns with me that they do not like to read. Comments such "I never read unless I have to." or "Reading has never ally interested me." are typically voiced by about a ird of the students in each children’s literature class I ve taught. One student remarked that she was jealous of her students in the class who talked about the pleasures reading because she herself had never felt any. Almost 1 of these students had never before encountered the :rm, but many of them have admitted that "aliterate" >plies to them. Although they are too new to be in the dictionary, the irds "aliterate" and "aliteracy" are increasingly being :ed not just in the professional literature but in the ipular media as well. Only a handful of sources have itually used these terms, but a greater number have cited .e problem of people who can read but don’t, using such tbels as "reluctant reader," "non-reader," "illiterate iterate," "literate non—reader," or "non-reading iterate." Similarly, many policy and curriculum state- ‘nts on the teaching of reading have stressed the promo- lon of life-long reading habits and the encouragement of creational reading. The National Academy of Education mmission on Reading (1985), for instance, states: "In- easing the proportion of children who read widely and re. WI. tht :h evident satisfaction ought to be as much a goal of struction as increasing the number who are competent aders" (p. 15). Likewise, Thomas and Loring (1979) ite, “True accountability in education . . . mandates e development of students who not only can read, but who e willing to read widely because they value reading as a sitive and worthwhile process" (p. xi). tionale for Defining "Aliteracy" and other Related Terms It is fairly common to hear remarks that vast numbers "illiterates" exist in America. But we need to ask: at do these numbers mean? How do we define "illiterate" ’"literate"? Many scholars have noted the elasticity of Le word "literate." Hillerich, for instance, observes at literate has no universally accepted defi— nition. It may refer to degrees of proficiency with print, ranging from the formation of a personal signature to the interpretation of a written passage; or it may reflect no proficiency at all, representing nothing more than a duration of time spent in a building called ’school.’ (1976, p. 50) llerich also cites the contradictory meanings behind the lliteracy" figures. UNESCO, for example, in 1965, re- rted that 50 percent of the world’s population had been assified "illiterate." "Illiteracy" in these figures, #5 Hillerich, "may mean anything from one who has 3g rmal schooling, to one who has attended four years or as, to one who is unable to read or write at a level ne di ab ci te th at di 11' de Le he vi CE ea he CE essary to perform successfully in his social position" 51). This proliferation of meanings stems in part from the fferent standards used in measuring literacy (e.g., Llity to sign one’s name, time spent in school or profi- ancy with print). Another source of confusion is the idency to interpret "literacy" as a dichotomy, following a simple dictionary definition of "literacy" as the basic ility to read and write. The tendency to view literacy as :hotomous has persisted, even after the term "functional teracy" was introduced to define more precisely the gree of literacy needed for functioning in society. vine (1982) points out that "literate" and "illiterate" ve been used to make inaccurate distinctions about indi- l ‘ual abilities. A flagrant example of the confusion sed by the use of these terms occurs in the title of a ular book, Kozol's Illiterate America (1985). In an lier book, Prisoners of Silence (1980), which did not e as dramatic an impact as Illiterate America, Kozol efully explains the difficulties of trying to measure 1t literacy. He questions, for instance, the use of de levels as a yardstick, saying that nobody knows ctly what a grade level means; if grade levels were ived from tests, we need to know which test, where it k place, who took it, and what it tested. In his later k, he is considerably less precise in his use of the :m "illiterate," giving it a range of meanings. He ex— ains that he uses "illiterate" to refer to those who :arcely read at all,“ and "semiliterate" to those "whose ading levels are unequal to societal demands." However, writes: ". . . for the sake of unencumbered prose, I ll combine both categories in the single phrase 'Illiter- e America.’ This does not indicate a loss of recognition the spectrum that extends from the marginal ability to he at all" (p. 11). Taking this eye-catching but inaccu- te label "illiterate," he stamps it on 60 million people, presenting more than one-third of the entire adult popu- tion (p. 4). The problem is that, faced with this title, suspecting readers would interpret "illiterate" as the ability to read and write, according to the common dic— pnary definition, and fail to see the "spectrum" that 01 has intended to convey. Still another source of confusion is the way in which literate" has been used to refer to lack of interest in ding or to a decline in book reading. Jacobs’ speech aders--an Endangered Species? A Shamefully Illiterate iety" (1985) offers an example. The speech is not about iety’s inability to read and write or about illiteracy, stated in the title. The speech is mainly about people "have no will to read" or who "have no will to read ks" (these two notions are used interchangeably in the ech). rem 3Y1 We] we: Who mean thes Tchudi (1980) depicts the confusion surrounding the :ept of literacy in describing the future of Johnny and a, "the anti-heroes of every newspaper or magazine arti- on literacy" (1980, p. xii): If Johnny and Jane pursue the traditional track through the schools (and college), their story will pretty much end here. After their last diploma, their bout with literacy will be over: No more grammar, no more books, no more teachers’ linguistic hooks. They will settle into suburbia and watch a lot of TV. They will never touch a pencil except to write a grocery list and never pick up a book unless it has already been on TV. They will raise kids, and sooner or later, somebody will say of those kids, "What’s the matter with them? They can’t even read and write! (p. 74) Johnny and Jane (and their kids) 1111LSLQLE, minimally rate, or aliterate? Tchudi draws attention to socie- inability to make clear distinctions between these 215. To cite still another example, is Mrs. George ’5 widely heralded pet project as First Lady truly a sign to "erase illiteracy," as it has been referred to 1e popular press? Or is her real goal in promoting the ing of children’s books the reduction of aliteracy? tiling to see the distinctions between these concepts, 1y fail to understand the true nature of the problems Lce . On the surface, the relatively small number of writers lave used "aliteracy" seem to agree on the general ng. But a closer look at the ways in which they use terms would reveal some obfuscation. A publication enti mesc alit "ali who latt be I not read sper rela say amou has inst ing GViC Equa tior Paps read tYpe ate ’ legs itled Aliteracy; People Who Can Read But Won’t (Thim- ch, 1984), which came out after a 1982 conference on teracy in Washington D.C., gives two definitions for iterates": "people who can read but won't" and "people can read but don’t." The former involves attitude; the ter implies behavior. While attitude and behavior may related, it is important to recognize that the two are the same. People who have negative attitudes toward ding and who say they don’t like to read may actually nd a considerable amount of time on school- or job- ited reading. Conversely, people who like to read (or that they like to read) may actually spend a negligible int of time reading. The tendency to equate aliteracy with non-book reading also been apparent in the literature on aliteracy. For :ance, Mikulecky, Shanklin, and Caverley (1979), point- to aliteracy as the real problem in America, use as ience figures showing a low amount of book reading. iting aliteracy with non-book reading raises the ques- l of what the role of other types of material--news— :rs, magazines, and documents--is in literacy and why Ling of books is privileged over the reading of other 5 of text. . Because of the heavy stigma associated with "illiter- " Robinson cautions us from using it loosely and care- 1Y to label people (1990, p. 13). The same can be Sdi( thai try‘ con: ate: Lab: con: our must be real the bECl tion are Ser; den- thi: d about aliteracy. It has been called a “menace" worse n illiteracy, which "is all but wiped out in our coun- " (Thimmesch, p. 31). A participant in the aliteracy ference I had previously mentioned refers to the aliter— s and illiterates as occupants of the "social basement." sling and, worse, mislabeling have serious and damaging sequences. For this reason alone, we must be precise in definitions of "illiterate" and "aliterate," and we t exercise great care in how we use them. A comprehensive and clear definition of aliteracy will useful to educators. It will help clarify goals for ding instruction and help teachers better understand problems they face as they help students of all ages )me successful readers. aarch Questions This study will attempt to answer the following ques— is: 1. What is aliteracy? How should it be defined? What its indicators and categories? 2. To what extent are claims about aliteracy and its ousness as a societal problem accurate? 3. Does aliteracy exist among community college stu- 5? If it does, to what degree? What is the nature of aliteracy? 9 4. In the light of new, comprehensive definitions of '\ iteracy, what are the implications of aliteracy? .mitations of the Study The study has the following limitations: 1. The survey of community college students elicited udents’ reports of their reading habits, attitudes, and itivations. Since reading is perceived as a desirable tivity in this society, the tendency to exaggerate read- g activity or report attitudes more favorable than they tually are is inherent in studies of this type. In this udy, it was virtually impossible to find out if and to at extent overstatement occurred because reading behavior 5 not observed directly. 2. The interview procedure that I used in this study 5 certain features, one of which is inherent in qualita- Je research and others of which may be unique to the asent study. One feature that is inherent in qualitative search is allowing free and spontaneous interactions :ween the interviewer and the interviewees in order to 'sue questions and explore issues as deeply as possible. use of this approach created the possibility that during 2 interviews the interviewees might have changed their .ponses as a result of the interaction. However, this dy is unlike other forms of qualitative or ethnographic LN i" rese ipar view repc each amou the thei exam tior rev tOpi read inte COmm Sign 10 earch in that it did not include observations of partic- nts in natural settings. I conducted in-depth inter- ws with participants in different school settings and orted on the results of the interviews by summarizing h interview. The length of the summary varied with the unt of information that I received and my judgment of relevance of the information to the research questions. 3. In the survey the students are asked to report on ir perceptions of the origins of their reading attitudes habits. In presenting and interpreting the findings, I not intend to reach conclusions about the causes and acts of aliteracy. However, I consider their answers [estive of causes and effects especially in the context )bservations and conclusions made by educators about Leracy as a problem. nization of the Study This study consists of several stages. In Chapter 2, I ine literacy as a concept and explore its many defini- 15 from a historical perspective. In Chapter 3, I Lew the literature on aliteracy and related cs--reluctant readers, reading habits, and reports on ership surveys. I describe the methodology in Chapter nd, in Chapters 5 and 6, I present the results of the rviews and survey that I conducted at Kalamazoo Valley inity College (KVCC). Finally, I discuss the conclu— 5 and implications of the findings in Chapter 7. for Fir lit or edu def the ing witl prei defi and ate Pres Char hist supp €th CHAPTER 2 LITERACY AND ITS RELATIONSHIP TO ALITERACY A broad understanding of literacy is important in ulating a definition of aliteracy for many reasons. t, as a historical exploration of literacy would show, racy has received many different definitions. Whether ot and to what extent we see aliteracy as a personal, :ational, and societal problem, would depend on our nition of literacy. Second, the ambiguity surrounding term "literacy" has added to the difficulty of develop- a precise meaning for aliteracy. Related concepts such lliteracy and functional illiteracy have been confused aliteracy. Educators need to have a fuller and more ise understanding of literacy in order to develop a ition of aliteracy. Third, understanding how literacy iteracy instruction have evolved would help us evalu- he state of literacy and literacy instruction in the nt. As Tuman points out, "We may not be able to e any of these conditions readily, but without the ical understanding of them we can hardly know what to t and what to oppose when contending factors clash, proclaiming itself to be the champion of literacy" am 32! CO] ed< get em int 12 man, 1987, p. 172). Further, it is important to consid- new perspectives on literacy and their implications for structing a definition of aliteracy. Research in commu- tion, cognitive development, linguistics, and anthro- gy have contributed insights and perspectives on how 'viduals read, write, learn, and communicate. Paris and on (1987) have noted the diversity of this "explosion esearch" and the difficulty of integrating the find- . Nevertheless, the perspectives generated by this arch are crucial to our understanding of aliteracy. :racy: Multiple Meanings A historical exploration of literacy will reveal the ‘ meanings that it has had in the last two centuries. racy scholars and historians have stressed the impor- e of an adequate historical understanding of literacy. range of meanings attaching themselves to the term rac and to the related terms literate and erate is at once an interesting semantic phenomenon barrier to clear thinking about the topic," (1987, p. Robinson notes. That literacy and literacy education nd a high priority in this society is common knowl- However, as Tuman points out, "While there may be al support for literacy education today, it is never ely certain what is being supported" (1987, p. 169). raff (1987) writes: “Modern societies tend to show ti re 67 1e Cl iii no En me "L to 13 he importance of literacy without defining or understand- ng it. Consequently, literacy by itself is over valued and aken out of meaningful contexts, creating pressures that ncrease the difficulties of learning useful skills" (p. 3). "Literacy" is a relatively recent word. The first use the word cited in the Oxford English Dictionary came cm a work published in the 18805 (Ohmann, 1985). Howev- , the adjectives "literate" and "illiterate" have been .ound longer. Before the nineteenth century, "literate" aant "Acquainted with letters; educated, learned"; .lliterate" meant "Ignorant of letters or literature: thout education" (Oxford English Dictionary). The adjec- ves did not indicate "a line that divided those who could ad and write from those who could not" (Ohmann, 1985, p. 5). The noun "illiteracy," which meant "ignorance of tters; absence of education,“ was in use before "litera- .n In tracing the history of the usages of "literacy," liams (1977) explains that "literate" was an adjective mally associated with "literature," which came into the lish language from French and Latin (Latin "littera" nt letter of the alphabet) in the fourteenth century. tterature" meant “a condition of reading: of being able read and of having read" (p. 46). This meaning is cloe atuz defi poli in t with teac Fass Amer: read aPpli SUPP< liter new 1 have did r therm readi °°pyi 14 se to the modern sense of literacy. Further, "liter— re" was not limited to imaginative works, as it is ined today, but included historical, philosophical, itical, and scientific works. (Ohmann (1985) contends that the influx of immigrants he later part of the nineteenth century and the concern finding a way of educating them led to an emphasis in hing the rudimentary skills in reading and writing. (1989) describes the education of the majority of .icans in the nineteenth century: 'Except for the extremes of those who received no schooling and the very few for whom schooling was a fundamental part of professional training or an expression of elite status, the large majority of Americans received little more than the fundamen- tals of literacy and the rudiments of what was believed neceSsary to the exercise of responsible citizenship. (pp. 40-41) Thus, from its earliest known meaning of being widely and being civilized, a standard of literacy that was ied only to a limited elite, whose culture valued and orted wide reading (Goodlad, 1984: Graff, 1987), racy by the end of nineteenth century had developed a eaning. It became a word used to separate those who the basic skills of reading and writing from those who ot; literacy became dichotomous (Graff, 1979). Fur- ore, as taught in the schools, reading meant the oral ng of a simple, familiar passage and writing meant ng (Langer, 1987, p. 1). sr cc me sc 2a 1e bu an th Co of th in 15 Historians like Gerald Graff (1987) explain that a it in thinking toward reading, especially the reading of arature, occurred in this country in the late nineteenth :ury. According to Graff: . . . the idea had hardly arisen that the litera- ture of one’s own language needed to be taught in formal classes instead of being enjoyed as part of the normal experience of the community. Literary culture was already a flourishing part of the extracurricular life of the college and the general community. College and town literary and debating societies, college debating clubs, student literary magazines, undergraduate prize competitions, and frequent public lectures and reading constituted an informal literary educa- tion of impressive proportions. (p. 19) Graff describes the advent of literature as a college ect, saying that it "coincided with the collapse of the unal literary culture and the corresponding estrange- of literature from its earlier functions in polite ty, where it had been an essential element of sociali- on" (p. 20). The educated class, with its wealth, re time, and culture, was gradually displaced by the ess and industry—dominated class, which wielded more ore political power. Hence, the literary values of lite class became less and less tacitly shared. Robinson (1985) comments on the way "literature" has to mean "a set of procedures for interpreting a body ecially privileged texts" and "the specialization of oncept of reading to the narrower professional activi- criticism, a shift from reading as learning to read- s the exercise of taste and sensibility" (p. 489). t: 11 fc 16 ccording to Robinson, "it is literacy thus defined that nglish departments strive most energetically to institu- ionalize" (p. 484). (Chambers [1969, 1983] and other ducators have reasoned that literary criticism as taught n many English classes does not promote interest in the aading of literature and in reading as an activity.) The methods used in teaching literature in the nine— aenth century colleges probably did not encourage stu- ants to read on their own. It was taught with methods :ed in teaching the classics: through recitation and as a :01 for teaching such subjects as grammar, rhetoric, igic, and elocution. Graff also reports that in the lleges of the 18005, authorities discouraged the reading books because they "feared that reading too many books uld only encourage student unorthodoxy" and "create nfusion in the students’ minds" (p. 27). Instead, the e of a single textbook was promoted. Although oral :itation is no longer the today’s dominant mode of teach- ; literature, it is enlightening to learn where the masis on a single textbook or a reader could be traced 1m. One thing is certain: the instructional practices of 1800s in no way stressed voluntary or recreational ding. In his study of literacy in the nineteenth century, ald Graff (1977) notes that commentators of that period 1d members of the middle and lower classes guilty of not 17 lizing their reading skills in much the same way that terates are faulted today. Despite the literacy claimed by members of the society, commentators concluded that in practice literacy was insufficiently used. Its value was barely recognized; available time for reading was not seized. Individuals, it was felt need to be told to read, told how to read, and told why to read. (p. 294) As Graff has maintained, the influences of the old leges of the 18005 are evident in the curriculum and agogy of our schools today. The subsequent alienation of dents from literature, Graff charges, "was in some :ee a result of the system’s failure to make the rituals the intellectual life intelligible to those who did not aady presuppose them" (p. 108). Graff finds support in lolph Bourne who argues that “the old college education for a limited and homogeneous class. It presupposed al and intellectual backgrounds which the great majori- f students today do not possess." Bourne also reasons . . . the idea of studying things "for their own sake," without utilitarian bearing, is seductive but implies a society where the ground had been prepared in childhood and youth through family and environmental influences. (p. 108) In their historical exploration of literacy, Resnick esnick trace the influence in American education of d5 of teaching reading in nineteenth century Europe. sing Protestant-religious instruction at first and eventually civic-mindedness, these methods had as eir goals the "the mastery of a very limited set of escribed texts" and the development of "fluent oral ders" (1977, p. 382). With the rise of child-centered cries of teaching, stressing "the importance of intrin— interest and meaningfulness in learning and the intro— tion of standardized group testing during World War I" 381), a new standard of literacy emerged--the ability understand an unfamiliar text through silent reading :her than simply declaim a familiar one. According to Tuman, the teaching of reading as a ent activity was a major educational innovation. Tests :d to determine how well students were reading were eloped in response to it. With the growing influence science in education, written standardized tests started be used, beginning with the first intelligence test n to army recruits in World War I (Resnick and Resnick, ; Applebee, 1974). In 1918, a general intelligence given to army recruits used two forms: the Alpha for recruits who could read English and the Beta for those could not. Of those given the Alpha, it was found that ercent could not read well enough to understand the , a finding which alerted people to defects in the ing instruction. This led to the growth in the 19205 raded and standardized achievement testing. The first ardized reading achievement test, the Courtis’ stand- zed silent reading test, appeared in 1915. This and a: st qe ef li re 9a st 19 ither achievement tests that followed were never validated, [Dd examinations of them revealed significant anomalies Applebee, 1974, pp. 90-91); however, they formed a part of L testing movement that has remained strong until the iresent. Graff, Applebee, and other historians help us see the rigins of some of the teaching practices that have become nstitutionalized in our schools. Graff laments the way in hich, throughout the history of literary instruction, edagogical approaches have continued to exist long after he cultural and social reasons for using them have disap- eared. In Chapter 3, I cite the numerous literacy schol- rs and educators who suggest that such practices may harm :udents, making them unwilling readers. As Resnick and Resnick explain, we have seen a >attern of rising literacy expectations," starting with a eneral population that could not read, to a mass literacy ‘fort to produce readers who had oral mastery of a very mited set of prescribed texts through oral reading and citation, to a goal of silent reading for the purpose of ining information applied to the entire population of idents in this century. Resnick and Resnick contend that . . . this high literacy standard is a relatively recent one as applied to the population at large and that much of our present difficulty in meet— ing the literacy standards we are setting for ourselves can be attributed to the relatively rapid extension to large populations of educa— tional criteria that were once applied only to a 20 limited elite. The result of this rapid expansion is that instructional methods suitable to large and diverse populations, rather than small and selected ones have not yet been fully developed or applied. Further, not all segments of our population have come to demand literacy skills of the kind that educators, members of Congress, and other government officials think necessary. (p. 375) 'he new literacy standard based on silent reading empha- .ized private meaning and gaining new knowledge. With this ew concept of reading as a silent activity came the empha— is on measuring how well people read. After several ecades of preoccupation with testing reading ability and erformance, we have started to express concern about hether people actually use their reading ability. Whereas reviously we might have simply assumed and hoped that aople who learned to read would use that ability, now we re starting to turn that assumption and hope into a stated rpectation or even a standard of performance. Note, for instance, the following statements made in e last decade by literacy scholars. Writing about adult teracy, Harman (1989) comments: An aspect of literacy that has perhaps not been adequately emphasized is that literacy skills are only as valuable as the uses to which they are put. Universal literacy--itself an elusive goal--is not equivalent to universal reading. Unapplied and unutilized literacy abilities alone have no intrinsic value. . . . Policy, programs. and research should all begin grappling with the fundamental issue of literacy activity, in addition to their preoccu- pation with illiteracy. (pp. 99-100) milarly, Guthrie and Seifert (1983) write: 21 A certain achievement implies that a student has the capability for understanding certain types of material. The student has potential for learning, but these achievements do not imply action. A high score on a test or a high measure of reading ability neither guarantees nor pre- vents the student from engaging in reading activ- ities that will lead to knowledge, self— awareness, or respite from daily tensions. Although most educators honestly assume and fervently hope that achievement will lead to good ends, the real point of teaching is action. Merely to endow students with a potential that is never realized would be a profound error. (p. 499) thrie and Seifert (1984) reason that "an underrated, :entially fruitful criterion of education is readership." Having attended an institution in which learning to read is the central mission, do students continue reading for social purposes afterwards? A standard for teaching is that graduates will not only acquire the capability to read, but they will choose to read for psychologe ical, political, occupational or domestic pur- poses. (p. 57) in (1984), likewise, describes the "literate person" as . one who knows how to read, but one who reads: fluent- responsibly, and critically, and because he wants to" 1). Further, the National Assessment of Educational ess (1985) issues this statement: If our schools are successful, students will levelop the skills necessary to read a wide range if materials. They will also develop the inter- st and motivation to read frequently and widely n their own. (p. 37) rlish Coalition Conference in 1987 issued the follow- tement: at they [students] pe readers and writers, Tividuals who find pleasure and satisfaction in 22 reading and writing and who make these activities an important part of their everyday lives, volun— tarily engaging in reading and writing for their intrinsic social and personal values. (Lloyd- Jones and Lunsford, 1989, p. 3) According to Stanton (1968), "lifetime reading habits e the utilitarian and recreational use of reading roughout a person's life, with emphasis placed upon the ea that these habits must play a dual role" (p. 38). e expectation that students read for both utilitarian and creational reasons and find pleasure and satisfaction in ading and writing appears to be a part of the "pattern of sing expectations." However, the fact that some educa- s have articulated this expectation, even turned it into olicy statement, does not mean that it is a value shared roughout the educational system and the general public. 'haps with time it will become widely adopted and imple— ted. Many contemporary literacy scholars have elaborated on definition of literacy as a cognitive ability. For ance, Oxenham writes, "The most important use of lit— y, perhaps the most unexpected to those who invented ind, as we have seen, often the most neglected, is its ion as a technology of the intellect. Literacy . . . sen the major enabling technology in the development ason, logic, systematic thinking and research" .pp. 132). Likewise, Soltow and Stevens comment teracy, by implication, is a "mind-expanding skill, 23 hat it is valuable in information processing and aids the 'ndividual in the making of decisions and the sharing of ower" (1981, p. 7). The assumption that the acquisition f literacy bestows cognitive or mind-expanding powers on ndividuals has been questioned by some scholars. In their tudy of literacy among the Vai people in Liberia, Scribner nd Cole (1981) conclude that cognitive skills are not a esult of literacy but of schooling. Pattison (1982) is he of those who reason that the kind of literacy valued in erican society, mechanical literacy, does not bring about gnitive growth. He argues that "much of the concern about 'teracy in America grows out of a mistaken belief that chanical literacy ought to complement the acquisition of artain rational skills that make the good citizen, but the ,ct is that the mechanical habits of reading and writing e not sufficient for the development of the critical 1d" (pp. 176-7). Langer (1987) conceives of literacy not "the act of reading and writing" but "a yey of thinking speaking." She explains, Reading and writing as low level activities can involve little literate thought, and using liter- ate thinking skills when no reading and writing has occurred may use a good deal of literate thinking. It is the way of thinking, not just the act of reading and writing that is at the core of the development of literacy. (p. 3) Current notions of literacy have been broadened to de new technologies such as aural and electronic They are not restricted to reading and writing or ir tl'. we la re fr 24 print. Langer calls literacy "a way of thinking, " and ltung (1981) considers it a way of dealing "with words a social setting, not merely to read and write them" (p. 0). Tchudi writes, "A more satisfactory coined term ht condense ’language competency,’ including oral, nt, and non-print forms to create ’languacy,’. . . . In event, one can see that 'literacy’ can be defined only only of one takes into account the broad functions of guage in people’s lives" (1980, p. 15). An important dimension has been added to the concept literacy with the growth of ethnographic research on racy. This is the View that literacy is a social and ural construct that cannot be separated from the con- t in which it exists. "Evidence from cross-cultural earchers, anthropologists, and ethnographers remind us literacy is not a set of decontextualized cognitive ls," comment Paris and Wixson (1987, p. 45). And 2r writes, "The practices of literacy, what they are hat they mean for a given society, depend on context. are embedded in a cultural way of thinking and learn- Id although they may appear stable in the short run, re ever changing, reflecting the growing and changing f thinking and doing enacted in the population at (1987, p. 5). Heath (1985) has complained that I has viewed context as a variable, and not as "the >r learning, which it is" (p. 2). Heath (1980) urs ce} use ins acq ing for inv pan (Ka rem my, 25 gas researchers to recognize the complexity of the con— pt of literacy, to devote attention to its functions and es, and to understand that "literacy has different mean- gs for members of different groups, with a variety of quisition modes, functions, and uses" (p. 132). Comment— on the value of Heath’s ethnographic research on lit- cy, Bleich writes that Heath’s study "urges us to aban— the idea of literacy as a trainable skill and to estab- h the principle that attention to literacy in any of its ects entails attention to the community, the culture, the process of language acquisition" (Bleich, 1988, p. In "The Ethnography of Literacy," Szwed (1981) argues ' the need to study contexts, maintaining that literacy olves five elements: text, context, function, partici- ts, and motivation. He says: . . . what a school may define as reading may not take account of what students read in various contexts other than the classroom. A boy, other- wise labeled as retarded and unable to read assigned texts, may have considerable skill at reading and interpreting baseball record books. Or a student who shows little interest or apti- tude for reading may read Jaws in study hall. The definitions of reading and writing, then, must include social context and function (use) as well as the reader and the text of what is being read or written. (pp. 304-5) Literacy as a "life-long, developmental" process remek, 1988, p. 480) has also gained recognition in nt literacy studies. According to the National Acade- f Education Commission on Reading (1985), "skilled ar ir sc 1e st Ta wh da er SZ Sci ab 26 eading is a lifelong pursuit. Becoming a skilled reader ; a matter of continuous practice, development, and re- .nement" (p. 18). Neilsen (1989) writes: "Literacy, yond the rudimentary ability to encode and decode words print, is a process of learning to participate fully in cessary and personally important social, intellectual, d political contexts. It is a lifelong process of learn- to read and create contextual signs in print and in iety. Literacy has many houses, each of which we can rn to make our home" (p. 10). The notion that "literacy has many houses“ or that it a "plurality," consisting of "configurations," has arted to receive wide support in literacy studies. lor (1990) urges educators to study the actual ways in ch they and their students use literacy in their every— lives through literacy "digs." In this manner "lit- cy configurations" of individuals can be explored. ed (1981), likewise, writes: Indeed, one might hypothesize the existence of literacy-cycles or individual variations in abilities and activities that are conditioned by one’s stage and position in life. What I would expect to discover, then, is not a single-level of literacy on a single continuum from reader to non-reader, but a variety of configurations of literacy, a plurality of literacies. (p. 305) Langer (1987) notes the changes that definitions of ol-based literacy have gone through, starting with "the ity to read a simple, familiar passage aloud, to the at th mi an "f re sc Na li ri en 313 SF si in C0 pu er Cy th ta an fr 27 ability to answer literal questions about the passage, to the understanding of word and sentence meaning in an unfa— miliar text-~a progression from rote to functional perform- ance . . . " (p. 1). She adds that, while in the 19705, 'functional literacy, the ability to participate in the :eading and writing demands of everyday living in modern :ociety, was considered essential," in 1986, when the rational Assessment of Educational Progress undertook a iteracy survey of young adults, aged 21 to 25, the crite- ia for literacy Changed again--"not as a set of independ- nt skills associated with reading and writing, but the pplication of particular skills for specific purposes in pecific contexts. . . . This View marks an end to the imple dichotomy between literate and illiterate citizens n favor of a literacy profile, based in a variety of antexts and uses of literacy" (pp. 1-2). Anzalone and McLaughlin (1983) have created the term specific literacy" to refer to the designing of a special lrpose literacy activity instead of a general adult lit- 'acy training in developing countries. "Specific litera- '" is a progression from "functional literacy," defined as e "level of competence needed to meet day-to-day literacy sks" (p. 5). Functional literacy, according to Anzalone d McClaughlin, is ineffective for the needs of the rapid- changing Third World nations, which would benefit more om "specific literacy." To implement "specific 28 literacy," "training is planned around activities which do 10t primarily, or even secondarily, promote adult literacy. lather, training focuses on content areas related to devel— >pment which demand specific literacy skills" (p. 1). Writing on "the lack of consensus on how best to lefine literacy" because of the "differing views about iteracy’s social purposes and values," Scribner (1988, p. 72-73) argues that "ideal literacy" is a combination f all three: literacy as "adaptation" (emphasis on func- ional value in daily life), literacy as "power" (emphasis literacy as a means for human liberation and social hange), and literacy as a "state of grace" (emphasis on me special powers, e.g., cognitive ability, attributed to lose who are literate). She states that ". . . ideal .teracy is simultaneously adaptive, socially empowering, 1d self—enhancing" (p. 81). Noting that Anzalone and =Laughlin "have coined the term ’specific literacies’ to signate such special-interest or special purpose literacy ills," she writes, "The road to maximal literacy may gin for some through the feeder routes of a wide variety specific literacies" (p. 81). Willinsky (1991) has coined the term "New Literacy" to signate a broad view of literacy that is similar to :ibner’s notion of "ideal literacy." Willinsky reasons It literacy is not just a cognitive process but also a ins of gaining social and cultural power. He explains 29 hat "New Literacy" is an umbrella "under which I can ather and examine an array of innovations in the teaching nd researching of reading and writing . . ." (p. 5), inovations that he believes encourage not only personal avelopment and transformation but also social involvement 1d change. His notion that literacy should promote self- 'ansformation and social change is similar to that of man (1987), who argues that to be literate is to compose d comprehend texts that embody "new modes of being" (p. 5) and to gain the power to change oneself and the social 1d. motion of Basic Literac : Conse uence In addition to the general confusion over the differ— t meanings of literacy, controversy exists over which )e of literacy should be promoted. Many scholars contend Lt, despite the rise of new and more comprehensive defi— ions, the definition of literacy still being supported society is the basic ability to read and write. They that our schools’ prevalent mode of teaching reading as Lls is proof that we are promoting mechanical literacy, :ead of a higher form. Many educators are critical of this emphasis, which ' claim, as I will explain in Chapter 3, has led to the dem of aliteracy. I will now summarize the arguments f literacy scholars who have drawn attention to the inade— acies of this notion of literacy and to the consequences promoting it. Since the beginning of this century the public has ed "literate" and "illiterate" as labels that separate ople who can read from people who cannot. Beach and pleman (1984) explain why this definition is both sim- istic and problematic, by comparing reading with cooking. ey imply that there are varying abilities, uses, and joyments of reading as there are varying abilities, uses, enjoyments of cooking. The often-heard charge, "Johnny can’t read," is a little like saying that "Johnny can’t cook." Johnny might be able to read the directions for constructing a radio kit, but not a Henry James novel, just as Johnny may be able to fry an egg but not cook a Peking duck. In discussing read- ing in the schools, we must recognize that read- ing involves as wide a range of different types of text as there are different types of foods. And, to imply, as does the slogan, "Johnny can't read," that reading is a single skill suited to all types of texts does not do justice to the range of reading types. (p. 115) The emphasis on the acquisition of basic literacy has to a number of results. On the positive side, some erts claim that almost universal literacy has been aimed in this society. On the negative side, the focus Low to read has spawned instruction that stresses decon- :ualized skills rather than the total experience of ling. As Wilson states, a "possible explanation for eracy is that schools focus on subskills instead of on 31 he global reading act, an approach that might rob our hildren of some of the enjoyment of reading" (Thimmesch, .984, p. 4). Dixon points out that although basic literacy "fitted n era when initial literacy was the prime demand" (1967, . 1), it is too narrow for the demands of our time. A umber of scholars contend that the emphasis on basic iteracy has resulted in the neglect of higher literate (ills (Langer, 1987 ; Tuman, 1987, Pattison, 1982, Heath, 985). Tuman writes that ". . . the real threat to lit- racy may be that the verbal meanings in written texts seem ncreasingly irreleVant to more and more people who have 1e coding skill, but not the compelling motivation to terpret them" (p. 4). He says that "one problem we face that the rates of ’literacy’ may appear to be growing at e very time when actual ability of students and the ulation at large to read and write symbolic texts is in line, and perhaps most troubling of all, the two trends be related" (p. 170). In his study of the state of eracy in the nineteenth century, Graff makes a similar nt by saying that "a statistically high level of litera- possession may in fact obscure attention from a loge; litative level of ability to use that literacy" (1979. 270). He cites the case of Sweden, which in the middle the nineteenth century had a high literacy rate, based the ability to read well orally, but this ability did 32 ot include understanding what was read. Heath (1985) ontends that the focus on basic literacy skill prevalent n our schools does "not in reality provide access to the roadest possible range of perceptual and cognitive skills. f we push only literacy skills, we guarantee that the :hools will not take responsibility for helping develop Lterate behaviors [which she defines as a way of thinking 1d behaving]" (p. 17). Similarly, Harman (1987) writes: The problem of generating literacy consciousness poses a challenge possibly greater than that of teaching literacy. It is one that has never been squarely faced and about even less is known than the problem of illiteracy. The numbers of people who are capable of reading but don’t is as baffling a problem as the numbers of people who are unable to read. It is certainly possible that the two phenomena are linked: nonreading literates certainly do not inspire illiterates to acquire reading habits. (p. 100) Pattison (1982) asserts that not all Americans who can d and write use their skills productively, partly be- se their education has not equipped them to do so. most all Americans can read and write. Thus we seem to equal. But very few Americans possess discipline in the its of language necessary for its advantageous use, and se who do effectively control the many who do not " 82, p. 181). Pattison contends that two kinds of lit- cy are fostered by our schools: a high literate training leaders (students in high track classes or elite insti- ions) and mechanical literacy for followers (students in track classes or community colleges.) (19: SGCC Robi "has dent memb thei rema lite] Writ. Prom< the ( haVe attit Chap empha Vatic about Prob] 33 This view is shared by Goodlad (1984) and Robinson 1985). Goodlad has found that in most elementary and econdary school language arts programs, . . lower track classes tended to emphasize the mechanics of English usage, whereas high track classes were likely to stress the intellectual skills of analysis, evaluation, and judgment especially through literature. The low track classes were unlikely to encounter the high status knowledge dealt with in the upper tracks and normally considered essential for college admission. (p. 205) >binson argues that stressing literacy as basic skills [as harmed all students much, but most of all those stu- ents whom we characterize as most needful of help. . . . mbers of racial, social, and linguistic minorities, for eir children most numerously populate our lower track and medial classes" (1985, p. 485). Scholars have explained the consequences of defining eracy as the basic or mechanical skills of reading and 'ting. The issue of which concept of literacy is being moted, basic literacy or a higher literacy, impinges on concept of aliteracy. Many scholars and educators who e written about aliteracy, reading reluctance, and itudes toward reading (topics to be reviewed in the next pter) assert that still another consequence of the hasis on basic literacy is a loss of interest and moti- ion to read--in a word, aliteracy. Moreover, decisions ut the nature of aliteracy——what it is, whether it is a blem and, if it is, how serious--depend in part on 34 one’s definition of literacy. If the prevailing notion of literacy happens to be the basic ability to read and write, once that skill is learned, then the goal of literacy has been reached. What happens after that point is largely inconsequential. However, with the new, broad definitions of literacy, what happens after one has learned the basics of reading and writing is enormously important. As Tchudi explains, literacy is "a process of discovering and know- ing," and "The fully literate person uses language in every part of his or her life, not just as a tool of commu- nication, but as a medium for knowing “ (p. 75). Literacy, according to Tuman (1987) and Willinsky (1991), is a power- ful tool. In their broad definition, to be literate, is to have the capability to change ourselves and our society. On the basis of these definitions, we have to conclude that the gse of one's literacy is critical to the individual and to society. Literacy and Schooling The relationship between literacy and schooling has been explored by researchers and scholars. Cook-Gumperz {1986), for example, states that although people tend to :hink of literacy as a product of schooling, historically .iteracy came first. Before mass schooling, literacy used .0 be “pluralistic," "home—based," and "hard-to-estimate." .ook-Gumperz explains that in the eighteenth century, there 35 was a “multiplicity of literacies," "a composite of differ- ent skills related to reading and writing for many differ- ent purposes and section of a society’s population" (p. 22). However, in this century literacy began to be associ- ated with a "single, standardized sghgglgg_lit§ragy" (p. 22), which Cook-Gumperz describes as "a system of decontex- tualized knowledge validated through test performances" (p. 41). Our current notion of literacy as consisting of many literacies seems to be a return to the old definition of literacy, as she has explained it. Maintaining that literacy had social and recreation- al value for people, Gumperz-Cook cites Robert Altick’s 1957 study of mass reading, which gives examples of how workers in earlier times incorporated reading into their work life-~for instance, a good reader would read to other workers while they worked (p. 24). In the twentieth cen- tury, "the popular cultural force of literacy was brought under the control of schooling" (p. 29). "Schooled litera- gy was thus differentiated from everyday uses of literacy. What was learnt through schooled literacy was no longer a part of a local common culture, so that ordinary people had less control over their own cultural products" (p. 31). urthermore, schooled literacy using testing created "a new ivision in society, between the educated and the uneducat- d (or sChooled and unschooled); and a new form of increas- 'ngly powerful control which could be exerted through the t L 11 n n u: m at te "t ra ev 1e di re 36 curriculum" (p. 27). (Pattison [1982], Goodlad [1984] and Robinson [1985], cited earlier in this chapter, discuss some ways in which the schools, through curriculum and pedagogy, exert control, favoring some groups over others.) Following a similar vein, Willinsky (1991) describes the "popular literacy" that followed the invention of the printing press, a literacy that “predates the emergence of systematic schooling" (p. 177). Willinsky traces the beginnings of New Literacy (which is explained earlier in this chapter) to the popular literacy of earlier centuries. Like Cook-Gumperz, he points out the effects of schooled literacy, but he goes a step further by advocating a set of new approaches to literacy instruction and by encouraging "a form of reschooling" (rather than "deschooling," a term used by Ivan Illich). His work summarizes the ideas of many literacy scholars and educators who have written about the negative effects of the "bottom-up" approach to teaching reading and writing and who advocate a holistic "top-down" model (pp. 68-69). Educators have expressed concern about the gulf sepa- ating school reading from the real functions of reading in veryday life and about the effects of this separation on earners. Purves (1984), for instance, describes some of ifferences between reading as taught in the schools and eading as experienced outside of school: . . an individual outside of school may pur— chase or borrow a novel, read it, and put it down r a 90 37 or perhaps recommend it to a friend, saying something like, "You ought to read this; it is good." In school that statement would not be accepted as proof that the individual has read and understood the novel. Reading the novel in school involves a series of complex activities before, during, and after reading to demonstrate something called comprehension or appreciation or understanding. The individual must be prepared to answer oral or written questions about the content, structure, or implications of the text, must be prepared to produce oral, dramatic, or cinematic reenactment of the different types of text on demand, shift subject matter and style every 45 minutes or so, have the reading inter- rupted, and particularly read texts that deal with subjects of little interest written in style that may be opaque or downright incomprehensible. I remember observing a class of high school sophomores and being told that two girls "could— n’t read." The class was dealing with contempo- rary poetry; the two girls sat in the back read— ing a magazine about film stars and discussing an article intelligently. They could read but they could not or would not do school reading. (pp. 82-83) ' lere Purves implies that school reading has standards and xpectations quite different from those in real life. He oncludes that although they can competently read materials f their own choosing, the girls "could not or would not 0 school reading." In an article "In search of the ’good’ reader," asinski (1989) contrasts the definitions of the "good eader" for students and for adults. For students, a "good eader" is usually defined in terms of a level of reading roficiency and later in terms of ability to use reading to earn in the various subject areas. However, these stand- :ds are abandoned when students become adults. To be a >od adult reader, according to Razinski, is to be "active, te ti mu ha 01] re ti gr in W th 11 re in. SC llt voracious readers," to visit the library often, to read without being told, and to talk about what they have read with others. To bridge the gulf between student or school—based definition and adult or real life definition, Rasinski says that the ". . . ’good' reader in school needs 0 be reconceptualized to be congruent with the adult ’good’ reader" (p. 85). Heath (1980) argues that school approaches to the eaching of reading are incongruent with the actual func- :ions of reading in real life and that, consequently, we nust find means to bridge the gap. An ethnographer, Heath 1as added greatly to our understanding of reading in >ut—of—school contexts. Because reading habit surveys »rovide only a limited picture of reading habits, Heath ecommends the use of such ethnographic tools as participa- ion and observation in the lives of individuals and social roups. Her own study of the functions and uses of reading n an all-black working class community produced seven ypes of uses of literacy, a list that does not include ose usually emphasized in "school—oriented discussions of 'teracy uses: critical, aesthetic, organizational, and creational" (p. 129). To Heath, the value of the gather- g of such information as the uses of literacy in out-of- hool context is not to make judgments but to show that he extent to which physiologically normal individuals 39 learn to read and write depends greatly on the role litera- cy plays in their families, communities, and jobs" (p. 130). Heath has written extensively about the differences between definitions of reading developed by policy-makers and the actual functions and uses of reading in people’s lives (p. 123). In the schools, "a student’s success is measured by a sequenced move through the hierarchy of skills and it is believed that acquiring these skills, i.e., learning to read, is necessary before a student is reading to learn" (p. stresses reading to learn at the same time that students 131). A view of literacy that are learning to read is an alternative to this approach, according to Heath. She provides an example of a first grade teacher whose teaching is governed by this philosophy that he or she explains to children: Reading and writing are things you do all the time——at home, on the bus, riding your bike, at the barber shop. You can read and you do everyday before you ever come to school. You can also play baseball. Reading and writing are like baseball and football. You play baseball and football at home, at the park, wherever you want to, but when you come to school or go to a summer program at the Neighborhood Center, you get help on techniques, the gloves to buy, the way to throw, and the way to slide. School does that for reading and writing. We all read and write a lot of the time, lots of places. School isn’t much different except that here we work on tech- niques and we practice a lot-—under a coach. I’m the coach. (pp. 130-131) ccording to Heath, the "teacher’s approach to reading nabled these students to define themselves as readers and wrfl den< ing in 1 size tior know many as a some imp] lite asl recr ins infl tEac defi with writers by their community norms and to grow with confi— dence into being readers by school criteria" (p. 131). One way, then, to bridge the gap between school read- ing and real life reading is for the schools to use reading as a tool for learning. The definition of literacy implied in this approach is not a narrow definition that empha- sizes the mechanical skills of reading, but a broad defini- tion that recognizes the role of language as a means of knowing and of dealing with the world. Heath (as well as many literacy scholars today) has conceptualized literacy as a lifelong process of learning to participate in per- sonal and social activities involving language. Heath implies that home literacy can be broadened by school literacy through teaching strategies that stress reading as learning. Thus, though their homes may not support the recreational, aesthetic, or other uses of reading stressed in school-based discussions of literacy, students can be -nfluenced in using reading for these purposes by good :eachers. Some problems created by "schooled literacy" (as efined by Cook—Gumperz) and by the pedagogy associated ith it will be discussed in the next chapter. CHAPTER 3 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE In this chapter I will review the literature on alit- eracy and reluctant readers and compare these two concepts on the basis of what has been written about them. I will also review the literature on reading habits and readership surveys to determine the accuracy of the often-repeated comment that the amount of reading has declined and that the American society is becoming a non-reading society. My final goal is to construct an appropriate, clear, and precise definition of aliteracy based on a review of the literature. I will show the meanings that have become associated with the concept, noting where contradictions and ambiguities exist. I will also identify indicators or omponents of aliteracy. The literature on reading reluctance is more extensive han that on aliteracy, which is a newer and less estab- ished term in the professional literature. The concepts re similar: both have been used to refer to people who can ead with varied degrees of ability but who either do not r choose not to read. However, no attempt has been made 0 compare the studies on these two topics and to show how he concepts are similar or different. Some differences 41 42 do exist in the ways these terms have been used. For example, the term "reluctant reader" tends to be applied to school-age children, whereas "aliterate" tends to be used more with adults or the general public. Most of the liter— ature on reading reluctance, furthermore, draws attention to inadequate reading skills or disabilities that impair the desire to read or cause children to avoid reading. It tends to approach reading reluctance as a school—related, if not a school-caused, problem. On the other hand, those who have written about aliteracy have not focused on the level of reading skills but on behavior (avoidance of reading) and attitude (a dislike for reading or a disin- clination to read or a disinclination to read books.) They tend to assume that the level of reading ability is not a crUcial factor in the aliteracy of individuals. Most of the literature both on aliteracy and reluctant readers have a practical thrust: to offer suggestions and remedies after briefly identifying the problem. Farr (1981) defines the reluctant reader as a child ho "is unable to read and unwilling to try because of a istory of unsuccessful attempts or is able to read but not otivated to do so " (p. 3). Cianni (1981) refers to eluctant readers as those “who function at a frustration evel and those who have an aversion to reading." He 43 claims that "young people who can’t read (anything) are far outnumbered by young people who can read (at least some things) but don't" (p. v). Joseph and Wittig (1980) maintain that "the capable reader who chooses not to read has no advantage over the person who is incapable of read- ing" (p. 3). They stress the importance of promoting reading as a recreational activity. In “Motivating Reluctant Readers," Johns (1978) iden- tifies two major types of reluctant readers. There are those who are reading below grade level on standardized tests, have some trouble with reading and avoid reading and those who are reading at or above grade level, can read quite well but choose not to read. He presents school- related and home-related reasons for "students’ reluctance to use books for pleasure and for information" (p. 70). School—related reasons include reading difficulties and poor instruction. Among home-related factors mentioned are parental indifference to reading and excessive television watching. Johns stresses that the “end or goal of reading instruction should be to produce students who gag read and who want to read.“ He offers teaching suggestions for changing "the student’s behavior and attitude from one of pathy to one of self-satisfaction and involvement" (p. 1). Thompson (1987) discusses the characteristics of eluctant readers, citing definitions of the term given in he professional literature. Among the characteristics of 44 reluctant readers are a lack of motivation resulting in a "permanent non—reading habit" and anxiety about reading. A reluctant reader may be a capable reader who lacks interest in reading or-a reader who is hampered by inadequate read— ing skills. Thompson recommends strategies, such as read- ing aloud to children, to use in the early stages of their reading development. Nolan and Craft (1976) define reluctant readers as those who have a reading disability which makes reading frustrating. Consequently, they develop “personality maladjustments," including "a crippling negative self- concept" (p. 387). The article offers approaches that have been found to be successful with this type of students. In "Motivating Reluctant Readers to Become Mature Readers" iCasteel (1989) presents instructional techniques for in- creasing motivation to read and to help students develop a positive attitude toward reading. Howell and Sylvester (1983) address the problem of reluctant or unmotivated readers by conducting a study of reluctant readers in third and fourth grades. The children participated in an after- school program in which adults read aloud appropriate paperbacks. The results showed increases in motivation and reading skills. Holbrook (1982) reviews the materials in the ERIC system that "address the problem of reluctant readers: the causes and indications of reading reluctance, some broad c2: ‘ a": _.T _.__._.._._.—__.—_ —. .. . ..— — . . 45 approaches to reading instruction that will introduce children to reading in a positive manner and alleviate potential problems, and finally, sources for specific activities to encourage young children in early reading efforts and reengage those students already exhibiting problems of disinterest." Holbrook says, "Young people who cannot read at all are outnumbered by young people who can read (poorly or well) but don’t. The latter, who choose not to read, for whatever reasons, have little advantage over those who are illiterate." That "reading never at- tracted the student’s attention enough for him or her to develop adequate reading skills" may be a cause of reading reluctance. Holbrook states that one factor that contrib- utes to the problem of reading reluctance is that "learning to read is a risky business." A child who reads aloud and is corrected by a teacher in front of the whole class, feels frustrated and discouraged. Repeated experiences of this kind result in anxiety over reading itself. Other causes cited are the emphasis on isolated skills in teach- ing reading, inappropriate reading materials, and labeling of students by putting them in reading groups, causing them to become self—fulfilling failures. Several books by prominent scholars in English educa- tion have addressed the problem of reading reluctance. Two books that focus on reluctant readers come from England, suggesting that the problem is a major concern in that country as well. Written by Aidan Chambers, Relgggggt ._.— .._.— F—W-fi-.- .. l'z'U 46 Readers (1969) and Introducing Books to Children (1983) discuss the reasons why some students do not learn to like reading, especially the reading of literature, and criti— cize specific instructional practices that create reading reluctance. He writes, "Reluctance. . . occurs in those who have the ability to read without any mechanical prob- lems but have no inclination to read except what is re— quired by way of work or everyday normal life“ (1969, p. 4). The students might have begun school with a desire to read, but "a good deal of this willing response has been heavily damaged or at least disabled" (1969, p. 4). Among the reasons for reading reluctance is the "the belief that there are certain books which children, whether they like or enjoy them or not, must be made to plod through in a kind of literary pilgrimage." Chambers reasons: "But if they are made to do so without liking what they read, then how far have we (and they) gone? How much nearer have we taken these young people towards the goal of true literacy? Not far, I think, and we may have helped them take a step or two away from it" (1969, p. 66). Chambers maintains that "the most reluctant readers are also book-besotted readers" (1969, p. 13) because they have to read books in every school subject. As a result, "all but the most avid child readers will want to escape from books and reading in their leisure time" (1983, p. 37). Further, because books are used primarily as sources of information, students m I!-—' 47 learn that there is only one way to read. Chambers’ particularly astute observations about the causes of reluc- tant reading will be brought up again in the discussion of the findings of this study in Chapter 6. Literature on Aliteracy The emphasis on the practical and on giving sugges— tions to alleviate aliteracy is evident in the following articles, which define and discuss aliteracy as a concept very similar to reading reluctance. In "An Antidote for Aliteracy: Aliteracy--People Who can Read But Won’t" (Sullivan, 1985), the antidote for aliteracy is an ap- proach to literature that teaches the reader to ask ques- tions about the work so that he or she becomes more en— igaged and stimulated. In "Teaching Freshmen Non-Readers, the A-literate Majority," Tanner (1987) addresses the problem of "seventeen and eighteen year old freshmen non- readers" who "outnumber those who read as college students ust and as educated citizens should." He contends that hey are "the middle-American norm." They read strictly or utilitarian reasons, such as reading road signs and ppliance directions, and "they see no place in their lives or reading as an opportunity for intellectual stimulation, rowth, or enjoyment." The author argues that the teacher an help the students find ways to like reading. He gives one general suggestions for writing teachers to turn tudents into self-motivated readers. In "Aliteracy: What Cl 48 Students Can Do to Keep Johnny Reading," Decker (1985) considers aliteracy, ''the ability to read but the unwill- ingness to do so," a major problem in American society. It affects all socioeconomic classes. Among the social changes that have contributed to the problem are the influ- ence of television on reading motivation and comprehension, the changing structure of the American family, and state minimum competency requirements. Decker offers solutions derived from reading instruction in Greece and New Zealand. They include the deemphasizing of basal readers, of work- books, and of competency testing. She also suggests that the teacher model good reading behaviors and encourage written expression of ideas. An important work on aliteracy is Aliteracy: People Who,Can Read But Won’t. a report on the conference on aliteracy sponsored by the American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research in Washington D.C. in 1982. The papers compiled in this volume express concern over alit— eracy. Part of the intent of this volume seems to be to ncourage a more systematic investigation of aliteracy by aising theoretical and practical questions: whether alit- racy exists and if so, to what degree, what its nature is, nd what can be done about it. The work raises more ques- ions than it answers. In "Aliteracy and a Changing View of Reading Goals," ickulecky (1978) writes about aliteracy in relationship to 49 changing perspectives on literacy goals. He expresses concern over the trend to create a sense of a "crisis" and to emphasize extremes when people raise problems related to literacy. He criticizes the tendency to hurriedly create simplistic but politically expedient solutions on the assumption that any action is better than none. Mickulecky points out that concern with illiteracy has brought about an emphasis on the teaching of basic skills and a lack of concern over aliteracy. He contends that because of higher literacy demands resulting from changing job expectations, the ability to continue learning must be stressed as much as the ability to read. This means that aliteracy and illiteracy need to be dealt with simultaneously. Winkle (1988) cites statistics from readerShip surveys indicating that "people are becoming aliterate in increas— ing numbers throughout all age groups in our society" (p. 40). She also discusses possible causes of aliteracy suggested by the literature on reading: instructional methods and materials used in the teaching of reading and home environment of the child. A third possible cause is low reading ability. However, Winkle points out that "while there is support for the idea that reading ability nd aliteracy are related, there is more support for the 'dea that reading ability is not a w of aliteracy“ (p. 1). Low reading ability may cause a child to lose inter- st in reading, but it may not by itself be a cause for literacy. However, the less interest a child has in 50 reading, the more he avoids it; the more he avoids reading, the less reading improvement occurs. Winkle cites the heavy emphasis on basal readers and on achievement test scores as a possible cause of aliteracy. ". . . [M]any students find little or no pleasure in reading when it is presented as a series of skill and drill lessons year after year" (p. 43). Winkle also contends that research has indicated that the home environment of children strongly influences their reading habits, mentioning educational and economic levels of parents and family size as among those sources of influence. She maintains that research does not support the popular notion that television-watching has a strong effect on reading habits. Weaver (1989) argues that the "traditional basal reading programs used in the majority of our schools have focused on teaching reading as skills, instead of develop- ing literacy in a broader sense" (p. 3). Because the basal reading programs reflect a "transmission" model of educa— tion as opposed to the "transactional" model, educators have labeled students as failures "when we might more appropriately consider our programs as failures instead, iven the untenable assumptions upon which they rest" (p. ). Furthermore, the students who ch) succeed 'frequently . . . join the swelling ranks of the aliter— tes: those who can read but rarely choose to" (p. 5). K t __,_- ‘—* .h.~_.—-d-—n-—_.. .. 51 Without using the terms "reading reluctance" or "aliteracy," several authors have addressed the problem of students developing negative attitudes toward reading as a result of their school experiences. In The New Hooked on Books (1976) Fader discusses reasons why American schools have created "unwilling readers and writers" (p. 79). One reason is the emphasis on teaching reading and writing as "merely the means to the end of school success" so that once that success has been reached, children no longer see any purpose for reading. Fader also blames reading reluc- tance on "our failure to give students reading materials that touch their lives" (p. 204) because of our "exaggerat- ed emphasis on quality,“ on the rampant use of readers and anthologies, which he calls the "surrender of inspiration to convenience" and on the use of oversimplified and unin- teresting textbooks. Fader’s solution to the problem is the "saturation and diffusion" of reading materials attrac- tive to young people-—paperbacks and magazines--in every English classroom. Fader is also critical of the emphasis on reading strictly for information and school purposes. Smith (1983, 1986) has taken a clear and strong stand against practices that make reading unnecessarily difficult and unpleasant for learners, saying that “the only way to learn to read is with confidence and enjoyment" (1983, p. 20). He is severe in his criticism of "inept" teaching: "If we succeed at all, it can be reasonably predicted that the student will not want to practice what he or she has 52 learned or will do so reluctantly" (1983, p. 8). Smith decries the reduction of reading and writing to drills and exercises, creating students who "will only read and write if they are required to do so, and then reluctantly" (1986, p. 15). Smith further says: Instead of constantly testing students to monitor how well they read and write, it would make more sense to check on how frequently they engage in these activities or on what students think about them. students who leave school interested enough in the subjects they have been taught to engage independently in activities related to those subjects always have a chance to learn more. But there is no hope for students no matter how well they have ’achieved’ who graduate thinking that all school activities are a bore. . . . (1986, p. 263) Trelease’s bestseller The New Read-Aloud Handbook (1989) addresses reading reluctance and aliteracy, without musing either term. He points out that the "reading is jwork" mentality perpetuated by many classrooms can be h icounteracted by regularly reading aloud to children of all gages. He writes: "It is imperative that we let our chil- idren know there is something more to reading than the (practicing, the blendings, the vowel sounds, something more (to it than the questions at the end of the chapters. And 'he must let them know this early, before they have perma- ‘%ently close the door on reading for the rest of their dives" (p. 8). ) In "Developing Lifelong Readers in the Middle .i Schools," Barmore and Morse (1979) cite surveys by the National Opinion Research Center showing that Americans k 53 read fewer books than citizens of England, France, Germany, and the Scandinavian countries, that many adults in the United States have never read an entire book, and that "approximately ten percent of the population read roughly eighty percent of the books." Furthermore, they write, Many college students do not read one book a year, and many people cannot even think of a book they would like to read. Such facts suggest that we are not developing in our schools students who are enthusiastic, consistent readers as adults. Few if any children come to school with a nega- tive attitude toward reading, but an uncomfort- ably large number of students do not like to read by the time they reach middle level schools. The numbers are proportionately higher by the time a student has graduated from high school. (p. 75) One cause for reading avoidance cited is early school failure resulting from children not reading at the grade level defined for their age. Because they feel they cannot read, they learn to dislike reading. "Aversion leads to avoidance and skill development slows down or ceases" (p. 75). Another contributing factor is the students’ tendency to read only for grades or for approval of teachers and parents. "Once they are on their own the central motiva- tion for reading often disappears. Such students become the American adults who feel that reading is a chore rather than a source of pleasure or vehicle for learning for the est of one’s life“ (p. 75). The authors blame practices in the teaching of reading and literature in the middle chools, practices that create or reinforce negative 54 attitudes toward reading. They suggest specific strategies 'that will foster positive attitudes. Wilson (1985) argues that testing has a negative impact on students’ attitudes toward reading. She contends that testing "all but ignores the personal experience of reading——the value of readers’ personal responses and interpretations as they connect prior experience to the text to make sense of it and to give it personal relevance. And it is these personal experiences with literacy that are the very foundation of life-long reading and writing hab— its." (p. 13). She writes that the focus on the so-called "literacy crisis" and on teacher accountability has pro— duced "unwilling and uninterested readers and writers." In sum, these educators point to aspects of literacy instruction which they believe produce unwilling readers and learners. They criticize the aims and methods of read- ing and literature programs, criticisms that I will bring up again in later chapters. Attitudes toward Reading Because aliteracy has much to do with attitude toward reading, I will present in this section some studies on attitudes toward reading. Despite the widely recognized importance of the role of attitude in reading achievement, some scholars have noted the lack of studies to support this belief (Alexander and Filler, 1976; Fader, 1976). Purves (1972) has called attention to the need for studies 55 that "should not look at cognitive growth only, but at attitudes and reading habits as well" (p. 107). According to Alexander and Filler (1976), because "research suggests that attitudes tend to be ’unique, personal, and highly unpredictable,’" (p. 1) and "because the number of studies is limited and the findings are, at times, contradictory" (p. 64), it is difficult to make valid generalizations about the relationship between reading attitude and such variables as reading achievement. Mikulecky, Shanklin, and Caverley (1979) report that new means of measuring atti— tude have made "research in the measurement of reading attitudes and motivations more feasible" (p. 12). They cite research suggesting that the "student frequently leaves school with negative reading attitudes and habits that influence younger siblings and the next generation" (p. 12). To break this cycle, educators might focus on improving reading attitudes and ability, rather than on test scores. They conclude that "little is known about adults’ attitudes toward reading or how these attitudes might be improved to provide better models for the children and thus break the cycle" (p. 12). The research in read- ing attitudes and motivations by Mikulecky et al. has contributed a great deal to the present study. On the relationship between reading attitudes and achievement, the National Academy of Education Commission n Reading (1985) writes: "Predictably, poor readers have L11 56 unfavorable attitudes toward reading. What is not so pre— dictable is whether lack of proficiency in reading stems from unfavorable attitudes or whether it is the other way around. Probably the truth can lie in either direction" (15). Van Allen (1965) stresses the importance of building positive attitudes and habits in developing successful reading experiences. He argues that "if the reading habit is to mature and positive attitudes toward reading are to support reading practices throughout a person’s life . . ." (p. 6), then teachers should consider "the feelings and attitudes of learners to be as important as the development of skills . . ." (p. 5). Reading Habits and Readership Surveys All the surveys cited in this chapter have acquired their information by asking respondents how much or how bften they read. Sudman and Bradburn (1982) have pointed out that some behaviors may be overreported because they hre seen by subjects as socially desirable (p. 32). Some authors reporting on major surveys of reading habits have mentioned the possibility that because reading is generally perceived as a socially desirable activity, the people in the surveys might have exaggerated their reading activities iNell, 1988). Therefore, the results of these surveys thould be examined with this possibility in mind. Lampert and Saunders (1976) discovered in their survey if high school students in Massachusetts that there were 57 students who saw themselves as non—readers and who ex- pressed low interest in reading although they scored almost as well as "readers" on reading tests and received satis— factory grades. They conclude that "the labels ’reader’ and ’non-reader’ may reflect students’ perceptions of themselves, rather than measurable skill differences" (p. 34). They also report that among their subjects, "a varie- ty of attitudes and levels of reading skill can be found among a group of non-readers who typically do not receive much attention in research or curriculum planning: the skilled non-readers, students who can read well but who perceive themselves as non-readers" (pp. 34-5). Lampert and Saunders, however, discovered that "the 30% who said that they did not like to read books, do, in fact, read: 50% read the newspaper more than ten minutes a day, maga- zines one to two hours a week, and novels occasionally" (p. 35). In other words, individuals who say they don’t like to read may actually do a fair amount of reading, especial- ly when all kinds of reading are counted. Because students at the University of Wisconsin-Osh- kosh had been telling their reading teachers that they disliked reading, Taylor (1978) conducted a survey of freshmen in college composition classes to find out their attitudes toward reading, their perception of their abili- ty, and their reading habits and interests. Her survey showed that out of 492 students, 59.3 percent stated that .._._ ,. . 58 they enjoyed reading, 11.6 percent said that they disliked reading, and the remainder indicated they were neutral to reading. She interprets these results as "heartening in view of the previous negative input from the students. However, the neutral and negative numbers were larger than reading teachers want to find" (p. 11). She also concludes that "the students are reading more than was believed but not the quantity that teachers desire" (p. 13). Other small-scale studies have been conducted to verify the accuracy of claims that large numbers of chil— dren and adults are aliterate. These studies are generally limited to specific geographical areas. Heather (1982) interviewed 40 English school children five times in a period of one and a half years to find out what types of materials they read and how much they read. She summarized her findings by saying: "I found that young people g9 read, robably more than teachers think they do" (p. 4). She ites another British researcher (Merril Brown) who con- ucted a similar study and arrived at similar results. nly one pupil in the each of the two studies could be escribed as a "complete non-reader." Both studies had our pupils who were "non—book readers." She reports great ifferences in the numbers of books read from pupil to upil and concludes that "mean scores of the number of ooks read are not a very useful indicator" (p. 5). She oints out that "reluctance to read often means a reluc- ance to read English literature" (p. 7) and that "reading tak of" stu real eac] hen acti ( PP reat prei tha: tha the by the tha witi Che. SUI“ Sio eXpi 59 takes place all the time which most teachers are unaware of" (p. 4). Anders and Cardell (1978) surveyed junior high school students, college students, and adults in Tucson, Arizona on their perceptions of their reading abilities and their reading habits. They report that "the average member of each of the population groups perceived that they compre- hended more of what they read, enjoyed reading more, and actually read more than the average person their own age" (pp. 6-7). These researchers conclude "the amount of reading done by these groups was a good deal greater than predicted" (p. 8) and that "perhaps people are reading more than has been previously thought" (p. 9). They also found that "the more these subjects read the more diversified their reading became. This diversification was accompanied by an increase in positive feelings toward reading and their abilities as readers" (p. 9). The comment that aliterates exist in vast numbers or that they outnumber the illiterates has often been made without substantiation or reference to research findings. It is significant to note that the few who attempted to check the accuracy of comments of this type by conducting surveys of specific populations have drawn similar conclu- sions: that the problem is not as serious as they had expected or as the public has assumed. lI-a — —, ' ’ —' wax attitude toward reading, reading behavior, types of text read, reasons or motivations for reading, and reading ability. Aliteracy is pg; a dichoto— my, and neither are its components. For example, attitudes can range behavior Furthermo: behavior distincti< Conc behavior "Which co perceive amount 01 contend t ing/non-r person w] occasiona aliterate fests its ing and The reading I importam a higher reading_ reasons definiti( ins alon. we shoul 80 can range from mild dislike to hatred of reading, and behavior can vary from occasional to complete avoidance. Furthermore, while we need to recognize that attitude and behavior are related, we must maintain the necessary distinctions between them in defining aliteracy. Concerning the relative importance of attitude and behavior in aliteracy, we need to deal with the question: "Which counts more--attitude or behavior?" If people who perceive themselves as non-readers actually do a fair amount of reading, can they be considered aliterate? I contend that the degrees of liking/disliking and of read- ing/non-reading need to be considered. In other words, a person who shows either a mild dislike of reading or an occasional avoidance of reading may be a mild or occasional aliterate. On the other hand, extreme aliteracy mani— fests itself in both a very negative attitude toward read- ing and a total avoidance of reading. The range of texts read and the range of reasons for reading need to be considered as well. Book-reading is an important indicator in that book readers tend to engage in a higher volume of reading as well as a wider variety of reading. They also tend to read for a wider range of reasons than non-book readers. However, in building a definition of aliteracy, we should not consider book read- ing alone as some discussions of aliteracy are prone to do. We should consider all types of reading and count all reasons f for pleas ability t on aliter ship beti Therefore attitude . The behavio: ability-- needs to attitude range of develOpm. soning f their re Of that. study. esting, reading ing. The do, Am become a 81 reasons for reading, not just leisure reading or reading for pleasure. Furthermore, although the role of reading ability tends to be ignored or slighted in the literature on aliteracy, many studies have found a positive relation— ship between amount of reading and reading proficiency. Therefore, one’s level of reading ability may affect one’s attitude toward reading and reading volume. The possibility that these indicators—~attitude, behavior, texts, reasons for reading, and reading ability--do interact with each other and in multiple ways needs to be kept in mind as well. For instance, a negative attitude may limit the amount of reading people do, the range of texts read, the reasons for reading, and the development of their reading ability. Pursuing this rea- soning further, individuals who dislike reading may limit their reading to textbook-reading, and only a small amount of that. Their sole reason for reading then may be to study. Because textbooks are generally dull and uninter— esting, reading textbooks may reinforce their beliefs that reading is dull and unpleasant and that they dislike read- ing. The more they dislike reading, the less reading they do. And the less reading they do, the less skillful they become as readers. In followed this stt design a: ments an sis. Research Thi followin def era acc col Wha dei tic CHAPTER 4 METHODOLOGY In this chapter I will describe the steps I have followed to answer the research questions that have guided this study of aliteracy. I will discuss the research design and the rationale for it, and describe the instru- ments and methods used in the data collection and analy- sis. Research Questions This dissertation proposes partial answers to the following questions: 1. What is aliteracy? How should it be defined? What are its indicators and categories? 2. To what extent are claims about alit- eracy and its seriousness as a societal problem accurate? 3. Does aliteracy exist among community college students? If it does, to what degree? What is the nature of this aliteracy? 4. In the light of new, comprehensive definitions of literacy, what are the implica- tions of aliteracy? 82 hearth—l To p cy, this summary c represen literatur aliteracy and to 6 identify These 90. tion giv at Kalam eracy ex extent. Characte CY- The Prongs. a Precis W To provide a clear and accurate definition of alitera- cy, this dissertation has used a three-pronged approach. A summary of the literature on aliteracy and related topics represents one prong. The goals of the review of the literature are to determine the accuracy of the claim that aliteracy is widespread and serious problem in this society and to develop an operational definition of aliteracy, identifying its indicators, components, and categories. These goals were achieved in Chapter 3. Using the defini- tion given in Chapter 3, I conducted a survey of students at Kalamazoo Valley Community College to find out if alit- eracy existed among community college students and to what extent. I also interviewed nine students who had aliterate characteristics to learn about the nature of their alitera- cy. The survey and the interviews are the second and third prongs. Through these three approaches, I hoped to develOp a precise and comprehensive definition of aliteracy. Rationale for Survey and Interviews The choice of methodology for this study was governed by the axiom that the research must fit the research ques- tions. According to Goetz and LeCompte (1984, p. ix) and Kantor, Kirby, and Goetz (1981, p.295), decisions on whether to use quantitative or qualitative methodologies should be based on the goals of the research. Defining aliteracy searchers To disco‘ communitj questionr Resp give inf informatf used gui intervier give anS' ing for. provide question method e subjects The Senting relative Summari: Sallple i SUrVQYe communp would v Pared, 84 aliteracy by finding out how reading theorists and re- searchers have used the term was a necessary first step. To discover to what extent aliteracy is a problem among community college students, I conducted a survey, using questionnaires that students answered while in class. Respondents in surveys asking for self-reports tend to give information that is socially desirable and conceal information that is not. Other studies of this type have used guided interviews and diaries. The presence of the interviewer in guided interviews might lead a respondent to give answers that he or she feels the interviewer is look- ing for. Questionnaires and diaries, on the other hand, provide privacy which may encourage more honesty. I used a questionnaire for that reason and for its efficiency as a method of collecting information from a large number of subjects. The closed-ended portion of the questionnaire, repre- senting the quantitative aspect of this study, provided relatively objective data that could be easily compiled and summarized. It also allowed for comparisons between this sample and other segments of the population which have been surveyed by other researchers. Any similarities between community college students and the general adult population would validate the methods used in the studies being com— pared. a The key-inf01 sent the the inte: nation c understa tions of The in-< that cou naire. the pict findings Researe Th strateg cording Prevale "If a t tion t] Althoug a Sing] eDriche //Wt Cerned ings' t meanim 85 g The interviews of nine students, who may be considered key-informants (Goetz and LeCompte, 1984, p. 119), repre- sent the qualitative portion of the study. The purpose of the interviews was to get additional information and expla- nation of the questionnaire answers and to gain a fuller understanding of the reading habits, attitudes, and motiva- tions of some of the students who took part in the survey. The in-depth interviews provided additional information that could not be obtained through a closed-ended question- naire. The depth of information gained helped flesh out the picture and offer valuable clues on how certain survey findings should be interpreted. Research Methodology Assumptions This study has used quantitative and qualitative strategies for gathering data, a methodology, which, ac- cording to Goetz and LeCompte (1984), has become more prevalent and is a sound approach to research. They state, "If a theory is valid, it should be amenable to substantia- tion through a variety of data-collection strategies. Although many theories develop primarily through the use of a single approach to data collection, they are generally enriched by broadening the data-collection base" (p. 59). //While the quantitative portion of the study is con- cerned with reliability or the replicability of the find- ings, the qualitative portion is focused on discovering the meaning of the data and providing a valid interpretation ‘ ~ .....;,-__ - (Rist, 1! enable t1 an empat interpre by the 5 students answers particip simply i The searche Accordir few mod percept: into tl qualita Pure et tool co intervj Search, / Th enable( means , Called triang1 the Qt 86 (Rist, 1977, p. 45).; As Rist explains, qualitative methods enable the researcher "to get close to the data, to develop an empathetic understanding of the observed, to be able to interpret and describe the constructions of reality as seen by the subjects, . . ." (Rist, p, 45). By talking to the students, I was able to find out what they meant by their answers to the questionnaire, in other words, to learn the participants’ meanings from their perspective rather than simply imposing my perspective on the data. The acknowledgement of the subjectivity of the re- searcher is an integral part of qualitative research. According to Goetz and LeCompte, "Ethnography is one of the few modes of scientific study that admit the subjective perception and biases of both participants and researcher into the research frame" (1984, p. 95). Although the qualitative procedure used in this study cannot qualify as pure ethnography, it makes use of in-depth interviewing, a tool common in ethnographic research. This tool allows the interviewer’s perspectives to become a part of the re- search. /1{ The use of both quantitative and qualitative methods enabled me to cross-check the data gathered through one means with data gathered through another means, a process called triangulation. As Goetz and LeCompte (1984) notes ,ur ’ _,. triangulation "assists in correcting biases that occur when the ethnographer is the only observer of the phenomenon under in‘ of the u: the poss the rese §1£1§¥ I c on sabbe at Kalam a total Communi (Englisl to Read Childre: include develop in the other c ware (y 102), 1 (Law Er 8'7 / I under investigation" (p. 11). Another possible advantage of the use of both methodologies, according Rist (1977), is the possibility that, through the juxtaposition of the two, the researcher will "see new and different things" (p. 48). Surve ! I conducted the survey in September 1990 while I was on sabbatical leave from my position as English instructor at Kalamazoo Valley Community College. Eight classes, with a total of 219 students, participated. Four were in the Communication Arts department: Developmental English (English 098), College Writing (English 101), Introduction to Reading Skills (Reading and Study Skills 101), and Children’s Literature (English 242). They were chosen to include students at different levels of college, from the developmental to the advanced level. To ensure diversity in the sample, I administered the survey to classes in other departments of the college: Microprocessors 1/Soft- ware (Electronics 215), Principles of Sociology (Sociology 102), Introduction to Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice (Law Enforcement 101), and Human Relations (Business 207). Selecpion of Subjects In selecting subjects for this survey, I sought a sample that met the following criteria: 1. Different levels of classes, from developmen- tal to advanced. 2. tecl sur 5. sur vie Cor Subject: 17, 199( Pilot St A I ter to ' questio then ir negativ parts c analysi t0 ansv tions, qUesti Studen Whethe] whethe approa. Was re 88 2. Different programs, including business and technical areas. 3. A mixture of males and females and of ages. 4. Instructors’ permission to administer the survey during class time. 5. Students’ consent to participate in the survey and, if asked, to be available for inter- views after completing the questionnaire. Consent for the study was obtained from the Human Subjects Committee at Michigan State University on April 17, 1990. Pilot Study A pilot study was conducted in the spring 1990 semes- ter to test the initial questionnaire I had developed. The questionnaire was given to an English class at KVCC. I then interviewed two students in the class who indicated negative attitudes toward reading. After the pilot study, parts of the questionnaire were revised to permit easier analysis of the data. Open-ended questions asking students to answer in essay form were changed to closed-ended ques- tions, giving them a list of answers to choose from. The questions that were changed were questions that asked students to evaluate themselves as readers, to explain whether reading is easy or difficult for them, and to state whether they think reading is important. Through this approach, the number of open-ended, essay-type questions was reduced from four to one, allowing for more efficient analysis ty of me survey v informat view set to inte Construe Th: percept attitud. or mo: abiliti led me tions 0 in the Nation; (Sudman Stateme were a< tude M( 58-61)' ments, Suitab Varietj readin throng: 89 analysis of the results. Questions on variety and intensi- ty of motivations taken from the Mikulecky et al. (1979) survey were also added. Realizing the range and depth of information that students can reveal in an informal inter- view setting, I decided to increase the number of students to interview, from four that I had planned, to nine. Construction of the Questionnaire (See Appendix A) The purpose of the survey was to find out students’ perceptions of their reading behavior or habits, reading attitudes, the types of materials they read, their reasons or motivations for reading, and their reading abilities--elements which my review of the literature had led me to conclude are involved in aliteracy. The ques- tions on reading behavior (what is read and how often) used in the present survey were adapted from surveys of the National Opinion Research Center and the Gallup Poll (Sudman and Bradburn, 1982, p. 58). Eight of the fifteen statements used in the Likert-type question (question 8) were adapted from the Mikulecky Behavioral Reading Atti- tude Measure (Mikulecky, Shanklin, and Caverley, 1979, pp. 58-61). (Slight modifications were made to make the state- ments, which were designed for a telephone survey, more suitable for a written questionnaire.) The questions on variety and intensity of motivations for reading, types of reading, and self-perceived reading ability (questions 9 through 13) were also taken from that study. Res: maintain desirabl would ht nothing gain the nation e tions, f relativ. Questio recreat then a: month, easier magazin Counter I askec neWSpaI title about. Q1 reader: and ne tion sDare b00k I 90 Researchers (Sudman and Bradburn, 1982; Nell, 1988) maintain that because reading is perceived as socially desirable, individuals who report on their reading activity would hesitate to admit that they have read little or nothing or would exaggerate their reading activity. To gain the students’ trust and willingness to divulge infor- mation about their reading habits, attitudes, and motiva- tions, I began the questionnaire with questions that are relatively easy to answer, requiring yes and no answers. Question 1 was intended as an ice-breaker, asking what recreational activities the students had engaged in. I then asked the students what they had read during the past month, using the phrase "Did you happen to" to make it easier for them to say "No" if they had not read any books, magazines, and newspapers (Sudman and Bradburn, 1982). To counter the tendency to exaggerate their reading activity, I asked students to name titles of books, magazines, and newspapers they had read. If they could not remember the title of a book, they were asked to write what it was about. Questions 2 through 7 are questions often asked in readership surveys. Question 2 asked what books, magazines, and newspapers students had read in the past month. Ques- tion 3 asked how often they read for enjoyment in their spare time, and question 4 asked whether they had read a book "all the way through" and, if they had, when? In questior types 0: read :35 these q‘ of this as dete T ure stt (state1 (statem f, g, l readin also a as rea they r Speed was in tions readi: Currer 91 questions 5 through 7, students were asked to indicate the types of magazines and the titles of newspapers that they read regularly (on a daily or weekly basis) if any. I used these questions so that I could compare the reading habits of this sample to those of other segments of the population as determined by other surveys. The statements in question 8 were designed to meas- ure students’ perceptions of the general value of reading (statements a and 1), personal attitudes toward reading (statements b, j, and k), reading behavior (statements d, f, g, h, and o), and attitudes and behavior concerning book reading (statements c, e, i, and n). The questionnaire also asked students to indicate how they rated themselves as readers and, compared with other peOple their age, how they rated themselves in terms of their comprehension and speed of reading (questions 9, 12, and 13). Question 10 was intended to measure variety and intensity of motiva- tions for reading, and question 11 cited different types of reading and asked students to indicate which types they currently engaged in. Individual items of the questionnaire were combined to construct the different components of aliteracy. Personal attitude toward reading was measured by adding the points for the answers to questions BB, 8J and 8K. The answers range from "strongly disagree," given 5 points, to "strong— 1y agree," given 1 point, and "no opinion," given 3 points. Likewise, self— erceived readin abilit was measured The men measure: for eac] "very 1. (indie: mm son’s 1 ranging combini ranging Q1 student reader enced Commer Studer perSpe dem0g1 educai race . 92 measured by adding the points for questions 9, 12, and 13. The number of reasone or motivations for reading was measured by using question 10, A through E, with 1 point for each answer of 1 or 2 (indicating that the statement is "very like me"). No point was given to answers 3, 4, and 5 (indicating that the statement is "very unlike me"). Intensity of motivation was measured by combining a per- son’s responses to question 10, A through E, the score ranging from 5 to 25. Types of texts read was measured by combining answers to question 11, A through E, the score ranging from 0 to 15. Question 14 is an open-ended essay question asking students to try to explain what had made them the kinds of readers that they were and who or what might have influ- enced their reading attitudes and habits. The written comments provided a fuller picture of the nature of a student’s aliteracy and its origins, from the student's perspective. The final part of the questionnaire asked demographic questions relative to sex, age, amount of education, whether a fulltime or parttime student, and race. Interviews Nine students who indicated through their question- naire answers extremely negative attitudes toward reading and avoidance of reading were chosen as subjects for inter- viewing intervi enjoyme1 for enj' or "st: read." A1 a fasti library were 4! ized ii an int schedu reques questi themse anyth: asked the q1 Viewi] thoug What thong to as they woulc‘ 93 viewing. For example, of the nine students who were interviewed, eight indicated that they "never" read for enjoyment in their spare time and two stated that they read for enjoyment "less than one time a week." All "disagreed" or "strongly disagreed" with the statement "I like to read." All of the interviews, except one (which took place at a fastfood restaurant) were held in empty classrooms, the library, and lounges on the KVCC campus. The interviews were 45 minutes long on the average. I used a nonstandard- ized interview format (Goetz and LeCompte, 1984), following an interview guide but not adhering to a predetermined schedule of questions. I generally started each one with a request for additional explanations of their answers to the questionnaire. As an opener, I asked them to describe themselves as readers, and I allowed them to talk about anything that related to that general question. I also asked them to clarify specific answers they had written on \ the questionnaire. "(One of the main thrusts of the inter— viewing was to ask for additional explanation of why they thought they were the type of readers that they were and what aspects of their school and home environments they thought might have influenced them. Another objective was to ask them how they felt about the amount of reading that they were doing and if they thought their reading habits would remain the same or change in the future. NY to test theref01 and pul interaC' I tried judgmen reading their 1 reader relati' brary, would I atmospl of the record Ed due immedi 94 My main goal was to probe for additional information, to test hunches, and to explore issues. As interviewer, therefore, I did not remain uninvolved. Instead, I probed and pursued questions as far as possible, and I encouraged interaction between the students and me. At the same time, I tried to maintain, verbally and non—verbally, a non- judgmental and neutral stance toward the students' views on reading. I wanted the students to feel free to disclose their real thoughts about reading and about themselves as readers. I believe that conducting the interviews in relatively neutral areas like empty classrooms, the li- brary, and the cafeteria rather than in my office, which would have accentuated my role as a teacher, added to the atmosphere of collegiality and trust. I tape recorded all of the interviews, except two, and transcribed the taped recorded conversations. The two that were not were record— ed due to malfunctioning equipment I recalled in writing immediately after the interviews. Data Analysis On the basis of their answers to the questionnaire, students were assigned a score for each of the components of aliteracy. For each component, the scores were grouped into high (for the top third of the range), middle (for the middle part of the range), and low (for the lowest third of the range). Using these groups of scores, I compared the scores of book readers on each of these compo— nents t extent I I person’ ments 2 survey, tionna. Additir the su the mi from t questi readin data f arrive concl' Howevi these repree Aisle quest liter ter ] exter 95 nents to those of non—book readers to determine to what extent KVCC students are similar to the adult population. I made inferences about the extent and nature of a person’s aliteracy from his or her scores on these compo- nents as well as from answers to other questions in the survey, for instance those in the beginning of the ques- tionnaire that pertain to reading habits and behavior. Additional information given through the essay portion of the survey was also considered. Finally, in the case of the nine students whom I interviewed, information gained from the interviews was added to the data supplied by the questionnaire to form a fuller and clearer picture of their reading attitudes, habits, and motivations. By combining data from these different sources, I posited that one can arrive at a profile of a person as a reader and draw some conclusions about the extent of a person’s aliteracy. However, judgments about a person’s aliteracy based on these data can never be final and absolute since they represent only a part of reality. Answering the Research Questions In Chapter 3, I described the answer to the first question, "How should aliteracy be defined?" from the literature on aliteracy and related topics. In that chap- ter I also answered the second research question, "To what extent are the claims about aliteracy and its seriousness as a 5 reports the Aml questi compar: studen survey nature I used C views. and 0; those 7 wili tion, 0f net 96 as a societal problem accurate?" basing the answer on reports of surveys of nationally representative samples of the American adult population. In answering the research question, "Does aliteracy exist among KVCC students?" I compared the findings of the present survey relative to the students’ reading habits to the findings of the national surveys. To answer the research question, "What is the nature of the aliteracy among community college students?" I used the results of both the survey and the interviews. Chapter 5 will contain the summaries of the inter— views. Chapter 6 will present the findings of the closed- and open-ended parts of the surveys and will integrate those findings with the results of the interviews. Chapter 7 will conclude the study by answering the research ques- tion, "What are the implications of aliteracy in the light of new, comprehensive definitions of literacy." Tl of the nity C: with n result interv T becaus charae read a enjoyr inter‘ use p: Profi \ mega; lute] He 1 ever: CHAPTER 5 INTERVIEWS This chapter and Chapter 6 will present the findings of the interviews and the survey of Kalamazoo Valley Commu- nity College students. After summarizing the interviews with nine students in this chapter, I will analyze the results of the survey as well as the findings from the interviews in the next chapter. The following students were chosen for the interviews because they showed through questionnaire answers aliterate characteristics: they reported that they didn’t like to read and that they never read for enjoyment or read for enjoyment less than once a week. Their willingness to be interviewed was another reason they were selected. I will use pseudonyms in referring to these students. Profiles of Nine Students Bob Although Bob, a 19-year-old student, regularly read magazines and a newspaper, he wrote on the questionnaire, interview, he explained that he had book reading in mind. He liked to read about sports and got Wed every week. He also indicated in the questionnaire that he 97 did a "I a week Countin read no these m books. Bc himself what h: the "s: magazi: to rea n’t do A averae averag hensii head learn get t stuff ester this eStat on re 98 did a "moderate amount" of newspaper reading. A few times a week he would buy The Detroit News on the way home. Counting magazine and newspaper reading, he said that he read more than two times a week for enjoyment. Outside of these materials, he said, "I never read anything else. No books. No nothing." Bob seemed to have a clear, unqualified image of himself as a person who did not like to read. When asked what he disliked, he answered that he didn’t like any of the "school stuff like books . . . ," but he liked sports magazines and the Reader’s Digest. He said, "If I’m told to read for school, I probably wouldn’t mind, but I would— n’t do it just for the heck of it." Although in the questionnaire Bob rated himself an average reader, average in comprehension and even above- average in rate, he admitted he had difficulty with compre- hension: "It’s hard for me to read and bring it in to my head the first time around on most things when I have to learn it." He said that he could read words, but could not get the meaning behind them. This applied to "school stuff" only. When he read material in which he was inter~ ested or of which he had some knowledge, he didn’t have this problem. For instance, he wanted to go into the real estate business so he had been reading books and pamphlets on real estate. He had no difficulty reading them. Bol novels zines. their rl As did, 80 heard 1 it. 8* future of rea pleasui class. for e1 respor and tr 0f scl answei ent p SPort each Subs< usual sOmet 99 Bob said that his mother was an avid reader of romance novels and newspapers, and his father read sports maga- zines. His brother and he had taken after their father in their reading habits. Asked how he felt about the amount of reading that he did, Bob said, "I probably could be reading more because I heard it’s good for you. It relaxes you. Still I don’t do it. Still I don’t read that much.“ He said that in the future he probably would read more but only "research type of reading like for my job. I probably won’t read for pleasure any more [than I do now]." Lee Lee was an l8-year-old student enrolled in a reading class. In the questionnaire he answered that he never read for enjoyment and that he didn’t like to read. He also responded that he usually didn’t read unless he had to, and that he thought reading was not that important outside of school. He appeared highly aliterate from his written answers. Talking to Lee, however, I got a slightly differ- ent picture. He said that he subscribed to two monthly sports magazines and that he read a couple of articles in each issue. He occasionally read Time, which his parents subscribed to. He read the local newspaper every day, usually reading the headline story, the sports section, and sometimes the classified ads. Le reading backs 1 he had er, hi Usually think, "Most I book h really he nev er, wh child: refere Arbuti Proble if he Paren1 novel below ardim was 1 read by re him t 0f h; 100 Lee also talked about paperback books he had enjoyed reading for English classes. He said that he liked paper- backs that are about 150 to 200 pages long. Altogether, he had about five positive experiences with books. Howev- er, his other encounters with books had been unsuccessful. Usually, after a couple of chapters into a novel, he would think, "I just don’t think I can read this." He added, "Most books aren't that interesting to me." He recalled a book his friends in junior high school had told him was really good--Phantom Toll Booth. .It was very thick, and he never finished reading it. (This novel by Norton Just- er, which is 256 pages long in paperback, is described in a children/s literature textbook as "heavily burdened with references that will daunt many readers" [Sutherland and Arbuthnot, 1991, p. 280]). Lee said that he had no problem reading textbooks; he usually understood them, and if he didn’t, he could ask for help from his teachers or parents. He said that it was hard to ask someone about a novel that he or she had not read. Lee considered himself below average as a reader. He said that he disliked stand- ardized achievement tests and did poorly in them. One book that Lee recalled having read with pleasure was The Pearl. His mother, a school teacher, got him to read the novel when he was in the seventh or eighth grade by reading parts of the first two chapters and encouraging him to read the rest. Lee’s mother read to him during most of his childhood and continued reading to him after he learnet read, ' He sai "If sr much a I mothel mothe: reade exten If it him, that reap: char: reco show: one ‘ the into caus trie higr thai 101 learned to read. She would tell him about a book she had read, he would read the book, and they would talk about it. He said, "I like this." He said later in the interview, "If she didn't do that, I don’t think I'd be reading as much as I do." Lee was one of six students I interviewed who had mothers who read a great deal at home. Three of these six mothers also tried to influence their children to become readers. Lee’s mother seemed to have succeeded to some extent in persuading him that book reading is worthwhile. If it hadn't been for books his mother had introduced to him, he would not have had the positive memories of books that he described. However, despite those positive experiences, Lee reSponded on the questionnaire with answers that would characterize him as a typical aliterate. The picture I reconstructed from those answers and from the interview shows a person who, despite of his perception of himself as one who doesn’t like reading and avoids it, has experienced the pleasures of reading and is struggling to turn himself into a reader. Lee said that he’d like to read more be— cause reading "might make me a better person." He had tried to read at least one book every year since junior high school, but he said, "I wish I could find more books that are as good as the ones I've liked." Tc to des mother read." about to pic one by liked "I car keeps me ti: read night just He sa that that don't his i you . text] that reme I do ther 102 Tom Tom was an 18—year-old law enforcement student. Asked to describe himself as a reader, Tom talked about his mother, "My mom is a teacher and she likes to push me to read." Tom explained that in the summer she would borrow about five young adult books from the library and ask him to pick one that he would like to read. He would choose one by reading the first five pages of each book. If he liked the way it started, then he would read it. Tom said, "I can only read a book if I can associate with it or it keeps me lively so I can pay attention ’cause reading makes me tired. If I want to go to sleep, I just grab a book and read it. But some books, you know, you can stay up all night and read. I like that, you know, if I can read books just like that." However, Tom said that he hated reading school books. He said that the reason he answered in the questionnaire that he didn’t like to read was that "most of the stuff that I read or that I’m told to read is school stuff and I don’t like to read it." Tom said he disliked reading all his textbooks, except for one law enforcement book "because you can learn information that is useful." But the other textbooks he called "boring" and “hard to read." He said that he had trouble concentrating on, comprehending and remembering what he read. "When I read out of a textbook, I don’t take it in real well. But if I read it out loud, then I remember it. Sometimes I have to go over it and over it said th to do, trate a Ir didn’t it does he sai force but I' look a once 5 read 1 the e broug guess more said, can 1 SCh01 idea read tion 103 over it and over it, so I can remember it," he added. He said that if he could tell English teachers what they need to do, he would tell them to teach students how to concen- trate and remember what they have read. In the beginning of the interview, Tom said that he didn’t like the way his mother pushed him to read. ". . . it doesn't work," he said. However, later in the interview he said, " I don’t know what I’d be like if she didn’t force it on me. Maybe I won't read at all. I might have, but I’m not really sure. It helps if she’s forcing me to look at this book, to read the first five pages, because once she has done that, then she doesn’t have to tell me to read the [rest of] the book because [now] I want to." At the end of the interview, he said, "If my mother hadn't brought me those books, I would never have read them. I guess it was pretty good that she did." Tom didn't think that in the future he would read more than he did at the present. However, he may read, he said, ". . . if I can find a good book. I don’t know how I can find them." Like Lee and Bob, Tom mentioned some difficulties with school reading; Tom had trouble concentrating and retaining ideas. What is the connection between self-perceived reading difficulties and their reading habits? This ques— tion cannot be answered solely on the basis of informatiOn gather: cussed planne KVCC. she ne subscr bough1 ads. poor poor She Came 104 gathered from these interviews. However, it will be dis- cussed more in connection with the next two students. Sue Sue, a 37-year—old business administration major, planned to start her own business when she finished at KVCC. She confirmed her answer in the questionnaire that she never read for enjoyment by saying that she didn’t subscribe to or read magazines, and when occasionally she bought a newspaper, she mostly looked at the classified ads. She said, I don’t like reading I think in part because sometimes there are a lot of big words, and my spelling isn’t that good. My comprehension of words isn’t that great. I've got to sit there and figure out how to pronounce the words and it just makes me think it's boring. It’s too much trouble. While she said she didn’t like to read because of her poor ability to recognize words, she also said that her poor spelling was caused by lack of reading. I know my spelling is affected by reading. I know my spelling is very bad, and I know that if I was to read more, I’d pick up more. When I came back to school in January, I was forced to read books and stuff, and I was reading more. My spelling was better because I was looking at the words and remembering them. And then after I slacked off this summer, my spelling slacked off too. So I know reading does affect my spelling, and I know it’s helpful, but it's just something that I have to force myself to do. She said that her reading ability has improved since she came back to school. Still she said, "It’s nowhere as good as I WI it use T readir "When taught basica have ' and 11 she w teach! she s didn' magaz it n! SCho< one-. came elem, and I co that exam [he] inte She 105 as I would like it to be, but it's a little bit better than it used to be." The reason she cited for her present attitude toward reading was that she never read as a child. She said, "When I was a child I was never enforced, or asked to, or taught to just read. I can read, but I don’t like to read basically. . . . it’s just like anything else in life, you have to make a habit of it, whether you’re young or old, and it’s just something I never picked up." She said that she wasn’t encouraged or discouraged by any particular teacher. "This is a problem I’ve always had in my life," she said. "I did it [reading] in school just enough, but I didn’t do any outside reading at home or free reading of magazines [and] books. So I think that’s why I don’t like it now because I wasn’t made to do it as a child." At school she said, "I enjoy doing things with my hands or one-on—one than I have reading about things, so when it came to reading I didn’t comprehend as well as I did." In contrast, her 17-year-old son, when he was in elementary school "loved to read. You’d give him a book, and he would just read, read, read. He was very smart, and I could see that his grades reflected his reading. I know that it’s very important to read just by watching-his example, and I know it’s very important for the young girls [her two younger daughters, ages 5 and 8] to read and be interested in reading." Knowing the importance of reading, she had tried to encourage her children by reading to them and DU' refer: just c .._1¢vro-rnnrfI—-I She readi night was k "I p] need. more busi WES Year She Coul tha joy not 80m 106 and buying them a set of The World Book Encyclopedia. She referred to the encyclopedias as "a thousand dollar set just collecting dust." She said, I give the kids the books and say, go look through them, but as far as me taking the time off to read to them stories, I know I have to start young by reading to them in order to help them when they get old to enjoy reading more, but when I start reading to them and they start fidgeting, then I just close the book and stop reading. She felt that she had a "tendency to discourage" their reading because she didn’t read to them as much as they might want or need. Sue seemed to read only for utilitarian reasons. She was keeping her textbooks, instead of selling them, because "I plan on going back to them and getting the information I need." She thought that her future would require of her more reading of this type. She would need to read more business-related brochures and pamphlets. When asked if there was any time in her life when she was interested in reading, she described an experience ten years ago when she was a secretary and was "totally bored." She explained, " I had nothing to do, so the only thing I could do was to read. So I happened to pick up a book and I got interested in it and I couldn’t put it down. And that was one of the very few times in my life that I en- joyed reading. Only when there's no other option, there’s nothing else to do, so I was forced to concentrate on something like that, then I find myself reading." She said that t intere: type A stirs chapte down. respo: leisu1 ronme1 jail, be my the 1 score scale (on a in t and self aver rang hair and fir: dur dau. 107 that that experience "made me think I could probably be interested in something. I'm an impatient person myself, a type A personality. And unless I can find something that stirs my interest right away, if I have to read through a chapter or two to get my interest [going], then I put it down. So it’s got to be interesting to begin with." In response to my question whether she would read more for leisure in the future she said, "If I’m in a closed envi- ronment with nothing to do, like when you get locked up in jail, then I’ll think of a book and read, but it wouldn’t be my first choice." In her questionnaire answers, Sue scored low, if not the lowest, on almost all the aliteracy components. She scored the lowest on attitude toward reading (on a 3-15 scale, she scored 3) and on variety of motives for reading (on a 0-5 scale, she scored 0). She had the lowest scores in the medium range for intensity of motives for reading and types of reading engaged in. Her highest score was in self-perceived reading ability (she considered herself average), where she fell in the upper part of the medium range. It is interesting to note that in the question- naire, Sue indicated that reading was not a useful activity and that it was not that important outside of school. At first these answers appeared to contradict what she said during the interview about the importance for her young daughters "to read and be interested in reading." However, in the readin: readin] busine busine E skill answe: readi] ates l choosl frust prono 1y, u langt woul. perce beha‘ lead abil Cycl com in c here the} to ; 108 in the interview, she did put considerable emphasis on reading for school. For Sue it appears that the value of reading was tied to schooling. Because she wanted to own a business, in the future she saw the need to read more business-related material. Because Sue rated herself as average in reading skills, I would have concluded from her questionnaire answers that she had no serious problems connected with reading and that she was one of those stereotypical aliter- ates who Can read presumably without any difficulty but who choose not to. However, Sue did say that she found reading frustrating because of her poor ability to recognize and pronounce "big words." (In the interview she spoke fluent- ly, using a varied vocabulary, and seemed at ease with oral language.) If she read more, her word recognition skill would undoubtedly improve. But, again, a predominant perception that she didn’t like to read affects her reading behavior. It is indeed a vicious cycle of poor attitude leading to reading avoidance leading to limited reading ability leading to poor attitude, and so on around the cycle. The cycle might have started with any one or any combination of these components. But once the cycle starts in childhood, it seems to go on into adulthood, leading the persons concerned to think that it will never change, that they will continue the cycle of reading only what they have to and of avoiding reading for pleasure. A the qt younge for s: [and] consi: rerea [than Althc that consi textt inte Sincc didn ment but airp beca i F l 390. ing. at . pap. 109 Ann A 33-year-old mother of a 2-year-old boy, Ann wrote on the questionnaire, "I never spent time reading when I was younger, [and] didn’t often get my assigned readings done for school. II don't remember any influence on my reading [and] that's probably why I don’t like to read." She considers herself below average in reading skills. Why? "I can sit and read, and read, and read, and when I get done there’s nothing there. . . . I find myself having to reread. When I read, I feel that I'm reading more slowly [than I should] and I’m getting lost in what I'm reading." Although she didn’t have much difficulty reading a novel that she had finished reading in the preceding month, she considered herself a slow reader, whether she was reading textbooks or reading a novel. Ann wrote that she never read for enjoyment. She interpreted reading for enjoyment as reading books, and since, except for this novel she just finished reading, she didn’t read books, she thought she never read for enjoy- ment. She didn’t read magazines and newspapers regularly, but about two years ago she subscribed to a magazine on airplanes. (Because of her dad’s interest in flying, she became interested in it and took flying lessons six years ago. She wanted a career that combined flying and market- ing, her major.) She read magazines now only when she was at a doctor’s office; she didn’t subscribe to a news— paper. I asked her if she considered magazine reading as readin ing i1 articl throug a few cle, curio arti chilc nevel since broug read went beca were put be c sad the}4 tim a c ree mot Sht 110 reading for enjoyment. "Only if I find something interest- ing in it," she answered, and she did find interesting articles to read occasionally. Usually she just "flipped" through the magazines. When she was at her sister’s house a few days before, she saw a newspaper and found an arti- cle, which she read "because it stirred my interest and curiosity." She expressed surprise that she read the whole article. Recalling reading only one book as a child--Qharlgtte;§_fl§Q--she said she started others but never finished them. The only other book she had read since elementary school was a mystery novel that her mother brought her. "It took me over half a year until I finally read it," she said, but she read it every night before she went to bed and finished it in two weeks. "I was amazed because I never thought it would catch my interest. There were nights when I was just real tired and I didn't want to put it down, and I forced myself to stop because it would be one o’clock in the morning. . . . It was really kind of sad that I finished it. It was over and I was real amazed that I had such an interest in it." She said that her mother read mysteries in her free time. She remembered her father reading to her when she was a child, but she didn’t remember any of the stories he read. She didn’t have any friends who read books. Her mother didn't try to get her to read, until last year when she brought her the mystery novel that she wrote in the quest mothe were stres mothe where 1n re more the 1 reme her her reat rea why cot Uni to: tal re: Di: 111 questionnaire she had read in the past six months. Her mother didn’t encourage reading when she and her sister were young, but now she sends books to Ann's son and stresses the importance of reading to him. Before her mother was married, she had her own television program where she read to children. With her mother’s involvement in reading, Ann was surprised that she herself was not a more interested in reading. Because Ann had indicated that she had read a book in the past six months, one would have concluded on the basis of that answer alone that she was a book reader (following the BISG surveys, this study designates those who have read a book in the past six months as book readers). However, that was the only book she had read "maybe since ghggz lotte’s Web," and Charlotte’s Web was the only book she remembered reading as a child. Ann repeatedly said that her being a below-average student in school was a result of her lack of reading. She said, "I think my feeling for reading is what affects my reading for classes because I really have to sit down and force myself to read. That’s why I don't think I get much out of it." Ann was taking a course called "Learning to Learn" at Western Michigan University. It taught strategies for reading and studying textbooks, and she liked it very much. She said, "I think taking this class and learning more about another kind of reading is going to make a difference. I hope that it picks up my interest and my skills in reading." R intere to ree year-l ing a hopir like she i enjoy pape recu read recc tha1 fin lau chi lea She not 112 Regarding her son, Ann said, "I hope he takes a better interest in it [reading] than I do. I know it's important to read to him everyday, but I don’t." Ruth Asked to describe herself as a reader, Ruth, a 30- year-old student, said that she wasn’t interested in read- ing and never had been. She read only for school. "I’m hoping that while I’m in school, I’ll start learning to like to read, maybe even love to read," she said. Although she wrote on the questionnaire that she never read for enjoyment, she said in the interview that she read a month- ly magazine, Guidepost and other magazines on Christian family living once or twice a week. She had subscribed to Better Homes and Gardens but she did not read the news- paper regularly. Because her husband was in the hospital recuperating from injuries from a car accident, she started reading a book written by Barbara Mandrell about Mandrell’s recovery from injury incurred in a car accident. She found that interesting, but she was frustrated that she couldn’t find much time to read it. She read the book between laundry loads or when her two own children and the four children she babysat for were taking naps. Asked what she learned about herself as a reader from reading these books, she said that she found out that she liked true stories more than love stories or fiction. who c choos adequ. anyth "A 1c call long she didn' for "enj remi read a wt and not a f: to: me. She 120 ree Say fh 113 Ruth appeared to fit the stereotype of the aliterate who can read, who has no difficulties reading, but who chooses not to. Ruth felt that her reading skills were adequate: "I read well. I don't have any problems reading anything. It’s just trying to find the time." She added, "A lot of people would call me active or hyper. I don’t call myself that, but I like to keep busy and to sit too long drives me nuts, so I like to run around a lot." When she answered the questionnaire, she indicated that she didn’t like to read and that she read less than once a week for enjoyment. For both questions, she was referring to "enjoyment reading, I don’t do enough of that." When reminded about the magazine reading she did, she said she read for enjoyment, one to two times a week, "but still not a whole lot." By "a whole lot," she meant "sitting down and reading a book of a couple hundred pages. That’s just not like me." Like the others I had interviewed, she had a firm and definite image of herself as one who didn’t like to read or didn’t read for enjoyment--"that’s just not like me." However, the book she was reading was 400 pages long. She had been reading it for about five days and had read 120 pages. The experience of reading this book made her realize she liked to read non-fiction and also made her say, "Like the book I’m reading right now, it’s just driv— ing me nuts. I'd just love to crawl up in a couch and finish reading. I'm interested in seeing what happened to her, i spare. repli: She were She book coui booi enj. her inf Chi the en: lea to 114 her, how it all turned out. I just don’t have an hour to spare." Asked about what influenced her reading habits, she replied, I think a lot of it was my parents; they never enforced sitting down with a book to study. We were never made to study so therefore I never studied. If I didn’t have to, I wasn’t going to, so I think that might have been a lot of where it came from and I find myself doing that with the boys [ages 5 and 9]. I don’t have them read enough. I don’t have enough time to sit with them to read to them. They enjoy being read to, but the 9-year-old has a hard time sitting down and reading a book. I think a lot of it goes back to your childhood-~what you saw in the home. My parents never read a lot. She added her parents read the newspaper but that there weren’t a lot of books at home when she was growing up. She remembered that they had National Geographic but not books. Throughout her schooling, she read as little as she could-~"just to get by." After high school, she read a book about a Vietnam veteran, a true story, which she enjoyed. This was the only book she could remember from her past. Like Sue and Ann, Ruth was concerned about how she was influencing the reading attitudes and habits of her own children. She feared that she was bringing up her children the way she herself was brought up. "I’m hoping that I'd enjoy reading because I’m back in school . . . I’d like to learn so much, and I know that a lot of that is just going to come from reading." infor were inter read" put 6 Mandr "And for w Wal 115 Ruth’s main motivation for reading seems to be to get information. Her two positive book reading experiences were with non-fiction books. However, at the end of the interview, she described a friend who "used to hate to read" but now read Danielle Steele books that she "can’t put down." This friend had told her to finish the Barbara Mandrell book, so she could read it, too. Ruth concluded, "And she used to hate to read . . . . maybe there’s hope for me." Joe Joe, a 19-year-old student, said, I don’t really spend a lot of time on read~ ing. Maybe I should spend more time. I know it's important. I have no enthusiasm to sit down and read a book. I've never read a novel. I just don’t. I lose interest. I think I just don't retain it like I should. And-that bothers me a lot of the time because I know that once I get to college--I'm in college--it requires a lot of reading, so it’s frightening sometimes to think about what I’m going to do, but I guess I’ll manage through it. Since he was in his third semester at KVCC, I asked Joe how he felt about the amount of reading he had been required to do in college thus far. His reply: "About average." I asked if it was manageable, and he answered, "Sometimes it isn’t because I work outside and it's not the fault of the school. It’s my fault, I guess. I feel that the reading requirements out here are all right." Living at home where he was helping his parents run a business, he wanted to move to a place where the atmosphere was more conqel inter him a selli ed il sure real clas opin know inst to i If) abOI qui: thr Se he 116 congenial to studying. Asked if he had found a course that interested him, he said that he didn’t know what interested him at the present, but he planned to go into marketing or selling. He was taking sociology, but he was not interest- ed in it and was just content to pass the course. "I’m sure sociology or philosophy is important, but I don’t really have to get deep into it, so if I can pass this class [sociology], that's all right with me. That’s my opinion." It appears that Joe valued reading in the abstract-~"I know it’s important." However, he could not cite any instance when he felt that reading was personally important to him. His main use for reading is "getting knowledge. If you read articles, you get knowledge." He was concerned about his reading habits because he anticipated more re— quired reading in his future college courses. Joe did not subscribe to magazines, but he "browsed through" Time and Newsweek in the dentist’s office. He also "previewed" publications about dogs (for his parents' business) but only to get ideas for advertising. He said that he read the newspaper to find out what was going on. "If you can call that reading for pleasure, I guess that's okay. I’m doing that to gain information about what's going on in Kalamazoo and other areas," he explained. From his elementary school years, he remembered a second grade teacher reading guperfudge. He liked it, but he didn't read other books after that because he was more inter schoo "were quire expla what scho< alwaj how that list read come tiox hen: Ask thi soc exl f01 em be 117 interested "in getting out in the playground." In high school he read TQ_Hill_§_Mggkingbirg and Animal_flarm, which "were all right." He read them "because they were re- quired. Otherwise, I never would have read them." He explained that he didn’t read the books entirely: "I read what was enough to get by with the assignment." A high school librarian once tried to get him to read books. "She always had a book and was always reading and would tell me how interesting it was. And we would talk about it and that's about it. But she tried." This librarian had a list of recommended books for people who didn't like to read. The books were out in a special cart and "you can come and take them any time." He never tried any of them. Joe rated himself a below average reader in his ques- tionnaire; he also rated himself below average in compre— hension but average in speed. In the essay part, he wrote, The reason I don't like to read is because I can never understand what I’ve read the first time through. Therefore, I have to read it again. and that is too time consuming. Asked what materials he was referring to when he wrote this, he answered "textbooks," like his accounting or sociology textbooks. He said that he could not recall any experience when he felt interested in the material and found it more understandable. If he had such an experi- ence, "it probably happened so long ago that I don't remem- ber. Like I said, I usually don’t read a lot of things." read. ’ schoc atti' “jus word by h plea to r to t inte pare homw he can enj tol pli su; re: gu' re WI 118 It appears that Joe’s attitudes and beliefs about reading had been based on the reading he had done for school. School reading consisted of textbook reading. His attitude toward required reading was that he would read "just enough to get by with the assignment." In other words, perhaps except for Superfudge that was read to him by his second grade teacher, he had never experienced the pleasure of reading a book. (I did keep trying to urge Joe to remember other books he had been exposed to. He seemed to have told me all that he could recall at the time of the interview.) His home life had not nurtured reading. His parents never read much, and with a business run from their home, they were constantly busy. Talking about his feelings about himself as a reader, he said, "I don’t want to feel that I’m illiterate, that I can't read, because I know that I can read. I just don’t enjoy it. I just don’t know what you call that." When I told him about the word "aliterate," he said that it ap- plied to him. At the end of the interview, he said, "I suppose if I spend the time, I’d probably learn to enjoy reading. If I had time to spend, I can probably do it. I guess practice is what you need, isn’t it? The more you read, the better you get." Ron Ron, a 36-year-old part-time student in electronics, wrote on the questionnaire that he "never" read for enjoy- ment that aver: so a scri arti larl 0th: ski] tio cou tar usi pie 1y br We b0 CC 119 ment and that he "strongly agreed" that "reading is not that important outside of school." He rated himself below average as a reader. I am this kind of reader because I never really enjoyed it. I always enjoyed playing sports or doing other things other than reading. I have learned over the years that you do have to read your required reading in school in order to understand and keep up with your class. For me to relax I like to just do it, not read and understand fantasies. Ron clearly felt that he didn’t like to read, saying so a few times during the interview. However, he sub- scribed to three sports magazines, and read one to two articles in each issue. He also read the newspaper regu- larly, reading the sports page first, then looking at the other pages to see if they are interesting. Sometimes he skipped the other sections and read only the sports page. Ron had defined "reading" when he answered the ques- tionnaire as "novel-reading." Asked why magazine reading could not count as reading for pleasure when done volun- tarily, he decided that he did read for pleasure and that, using the same argument, "everyone" read for enjoyment and pleasure. Ron named several persons he knew who read constant- ly: his wife, who read romance novels, his mother, his brother, and a friend "who reads about 3 or 4 books a week." However, Ron had read only two books in his life, both by Tolkien, and this took place when he first went to college (he quit college after a year). He said that he enjO] the t caus ques not cons and he < papa reac he The: fil rea as ing he sci re< sa bo Ch he ta 120 enjoyed these books, but he never read the third book in the trilogy. He had not read another novel since. He considered himself below average as a reader be- cause he did not read as much as other people. In the questionnaire he also "strongly agreed" that "reading is not that important outside of school." This answer appears consistent with his statement that he didn’t like to read and that he read only what he had to for school. However, he completely ignored the reading of magazines and news- papers, which he obviously did regularly. When asked about reading at work, at first he said that he read nothing, but he later acknowledged that he read computer writing. Therefore, Ron actually read more than he thought when he filled out the questionnaire. He realized that he actually read for pleasure several times a week, instead of "never," as he answered in the questionnaire. Ron's reading habits seem consistent with his upbring- ing, which stressed reading for information. He said that he didn’t recall anyone ever reading to him at home or in school and didn’t remember any children’s books. He did recall having magazines, newspapers, and The Encyclopedia Britannica around the house when he was growing up. He said that he felt "only a little guilty" about his-non— book reading, and he didn’t expect his reading habits to change.- He expected to get most of the information he needed from newspapers, magazines, television, radio, and talking to people. abou elec answ indi alsc the) sam] skii int of War sa: th. th in fi 121 Keith While the other interviewees expressed misgivings about how little they read, Keith, a 31-year-old student in electronics, seemed to flaunt his non-reading habits. His answers in the questionnaire caught my attention because he indicated not only that he never read for enjoyment, but also that he never read magazines, newspapers, and books. (He was among the 1.4 percent "non-readers"--those who said they didn’t read books, magazines, and newspapers--in this sample.) Yet he rated himself above average in reading skills. He wrote the following in the questionnaire: Reading is definitely not a priority with me. Reading is very time consuming as compared to other forms of information, such as televi- sion. I believe my reading skills are above many people who read all the time, but reading is not efficient so I don’t do it! Television or video programs designed to take the place of reading is a far better source of information and knowledge. When I first called Keith’s house to ask if I could interview him, his wife answered. I told her the purpose of my call and the topic of my project. When she heard I wanted to talk to her husband about his reading habits, she said that she and he always talked about his reading be- cause he never read and she thought it was important for the sake of the children that he read. Keith told me that he was just named vice president of the engineering company that his father owned. When he was in high school, he never took a book home, yet he graduated with foun rinq teac tic] dec; away tr01 ic. row the he whe ste was ex1 db ha in He re fi with almost a 3.5 average. When he started college, he 122 found his courses easier than high school courses. Refer- ring to the students in the class, he said, “It was like teaching kindergarteners." There was far too much repeti- tion, and he himself needed to be told only once. He decided then that college was not for him. Having been away for 12 years, he was back in college taking an elec- tronics course. In this course, his attendance was errat- ic. But although he had just missed three classes in a row, he said that when he got back, "I’ll know exactly what they’re talking about." He was taking this class so that he would learn to work with people he employed; “. . . when they’re doing something a certain way, I would under- stand." He didn't expect to pass the course because he was not trying to. To pass the course, he had to put in extra time to meet the laboratory requirements, and he .pa didn’t have the time. Although he bought the textbook, he had never opened it. Keith said, "I’ve never read a book in my life. That includes textbooks. I learn from watching and listening." He only read if "it’s definitely required that I read." He read technical information that he had to know. Now that he was vice president, he has to learn how to be a manager, instead of being solely an engineer, so he had been reading articles and booklets about management. "If I find an a1 what read. actu bein watc acti for on in to We 123 an article, like in the Reader’s Digest, that is exactly what I want to read, then I read that." According to Keith, there was no place for pleasure reading in his life. He said, "I think it’s a big joke to actually read for enjoyment. . . . I can't imagine that being fun." He liked to watch movies, but he did not watch much television. He didn’t consider himself a very active person. He liked to sit on his work bench and work for hours. ‘I call [it] playing around with a circuit. And I just want to see how it works, how it reacts to certain conditions. I am alone then» I want to work from seven at night to two in the morning, so that I can do that without interrup- tion. And that’s how I get the most work done. When he was about twelve years old he started working for his father. "He started me fixing some tape recorders and things like that when I was growing up." He described his father as someone who went to college and did the same thing he did. He thought it was a waste of time and didn’t finish. But, according to Keith, his father is "very good" and used to consult for Sony and RCA. "He is one of the t0p people in the video industry." Keith’s main interest is in toys. "After we sell this company, I want to design and build toys. Really neat ones. I've always wanted to do that." He had worked for two other companies as an "engineer." While he was employed by one company for two and a half years, he worked on about 20 projects. Two years after that when he went back and introduced himse find his 1 just degr ever gett was elec had answ yea EV SC Wa fa 124 himself to the men working there, they were surprised to find out that he was so young. They thought that, because his name was on many projects, he was in his sixties. Keith didn’t believe a person can become an engineer just by attending college. He would have had a college degree if he thought it helped. But he said, "I knew everything they were teaching me, and all I was doing was getting the grade." He mentioned a friend’s daughter who was going to Michigan Technological University to become an electronics engineer. He asked his friend if his daughter had ever taken apart a tape recorder or fixed it. The answer was no. Keith felt that when she got out in four years, this person would not be a true engineer: She’s going to know whatever was in a book writ- ten by someone who read whatever was in his books. It doesn’t make any sense. I think that's probably why I excel in what I do because I don’t rely on what I read in a book. I’m learning all the time. I'm still learning. Keith said about his three children: "Fortunately, for them, in this world, they need to not be like me [laughter]." The oldest is 14 years old and is a "straight A student." Keith told his son, who worked for four hours every night on his homework, "If you graduate from high school with a 4.0 average, I’ll get you a Porsche." Keith wanted him to go to Harvard to become a lawyer, perhaps the family business lawyer. I asked him how his reading skills could develop without practice. "I don’t know," he replied. He thought that when book: of t be h sion to ' more woul int] be nor lis adx Itl res hi: ab be to as at 125 that he read faster than his wife, who was a salutatorian when she graduated from high school and who always read books. He had written instruction manuals and other types of technical material. He said his spelling was good and he had been complimented for being able to write "profes- sionally." His boss at Humphrey Products used to ask him to "rewrite" the boss’s reports "so that they would sound more professional, with the big words. And I did. So you wouldn’t think I’d be able to do that." Keith is indeed very different from the others I interviewed. His reasons for not reading did not appear to be related to his reading ability. They seemed to have more to do with his preferred learning style: learning by listening and watching. In his field, he had been able to advance to the position he was in through that method. It’s interesting to note that with Keith taking on new responsibilities as vice president of his father's company, his reading interests had expanded. Now he wanted to learn about "new concepts in management." One could say that before this point, Keith felt no need to learn about other topics because he was narrowly focused on electronics. I asked him how he got information about politics and world affairs. "From TV," he replied. But he said that he didn’t watch the news regularly. "Like I said, I’m just lucky. I just happen to turn on the TV and there it is. The next five minutes if I watch it, I catch up on the 126 whole Iraqi situation." He said he also listened to the car radio, going to and from work. Keith appeared satis- fied with whatever he could pick up from brief periods of television watching and didn’t feel the need to learn more. Like the other nine students, for Keith reading was for getting information. However, while the other stu- dents indicated that the value of reading is largely tied to college, Keith had no use for college. So having no use for college, he had no use for reading. Unlike most of the other students who had enjoyed reading some books, Keith seemed to have never had an enjoyable experience reading a book. He said that the idea of reading for enjoyment was "ridiculous" and "a big joke." Most of the students expressed ambivalence toward reading: they didn’t like it, but they "knew" it's impor- tant. They felt frustrated by it, but they wished they could read more. Those with children wanted them to enjoy reading more and to read more than they did. The only hint of ambivalence Keith showed concerned his children’s read- ing. "Fortunately," he said slowly, as though choosing his words very carefully, "for them, in this world, they need to not be like me." And he appeared proud of his son who spent four hours doing his homework every night. He wanted to reward and reinforce the very behavior that he himself had said had no use for. view ics coll opti plat sys stul mat co: th al a1 ic' 127 I believe that Keith had such adamant and extreme views about reading partly because he was in the electron- ics field. His company made video systems for schools and colleges. A system his company had developed used fiber Optics and ran 40 video cassette recorders and laser disk players. Keith explained that an instructor could have a system like this under his or her disposal. He said that studies have shown that children, who already are "accli- mated to learning from TV anyway" learn from this. One can conclude that Keith’s beliefs about reading reflect his knowledge of and faith in modern electronic technology. Discussion These interviews provided information that helped me correctly interpret the questionnaire answers. Some of the information gained from the interviews significantly altered the reading profile that I had gotten from the questionnaires. Listed below are notions on the nature of aliteracy‘that I have derived from these interviews. These ideas will be discussed more thoroughly in Chapter 6. students’ Perceptions of Themselves as Readers All of these students had a strong perception of themselves as people who did not like to read and who read only what they had to. Actually, they read more than they realized or indicated on the questionnaire. Their tendency 128 to underestimate their actual reading seemed to be a result of their belief that they didn’t like to read. Students’ Views of Book Reading For most of these students, the perception that they did not like to read is rooted in their book reading expe- riences. They had defined "reading" as "book reading“ and, in some cases, as textbook reading. Although many of them had had a few pleasurable experiences with book reading, the experiences seemed to have been too few and far between to have made a mark on their thinking. The negative, frustrating experiences, mostly with textbooks, had made a stronger impression on them. School Reading School reading appeared to have had a strong hold on the attitudes these students have toward reading. The belief that book reading is dull, frustrating, time consum- ing--and that it is work--seemed to dominate their think- ing. This belief seemed to have blotted out the more pleasant experiences they have had with reading. Home Environment Many of these students appeared to be following the reading patterns set at home. Some mentioned the lack of 129 parental encouragement when they were children as the reason why they didn’t learn to like reading. Some re- ferred to the absence of books and the presence of informa- tive materials, such as newspapers, magazines, and encyclo- pedias. The types of reading materials they had in the home while they were growing up might explain why they mainly read for information. Some mentioned that their mothers read books, but their fathers read newspapers and magazines. In these instances, the children seemed to have been influenced by the fathers’ reading habits. Two students had mothers who tried to help them become inter- ested in book reading, and these admitted that if it hadn’t been for their mothers, they would not have read the books they read. However, the mothers’ influence on their read- ing was limited and did not turn them into avid readers. Other factors appeared to have affected their reading attitudes and habits. Reading Ability While these students reported that they didn’t like to read and never or rarely read for enjoyment, only half of them rated themselves below average or poor as readers. The other half considered themselves average, except for one (Keith) who rated himself above average. However, again except for Keith, all mentioned a "problem" in read- ing or difficulties in reading textbooks. Among the prob— lems mentioned are inability to concentrate and comprehend, 130 _ poor retention, and poor spelling. In addition to a misun- derstanding of the reading process implied by citing "poor spelling" as a reading problem, these problems may not be real reading problems. They may be normal difficulties associated with having to read material that is poorly written or that is uninteresting and unfamiliar to the reader. In other words, the problem might lie mostly in the text and not as much in the reader. Reasons for Reading When they read, these students read for information. They did not see themselves as readers for pleasure. Hence, even those who read magazines and/or newspapers at least once a week, reported that they never read for enjoyment. Students’ Attitudes Toward Their Reading Habits Most of the students reported that their reading habits and attitudes had a negative impact on their lives. They said that their school work and grades have suffered because they didn’t like to read their textbooks. All, except for one, stated that they believed reading is impor- tant and that it is important to read. Hence, they ex— pressed various degrees of discomfort about their percep- tion that they didn’t like to read. The wished that they read more, wished that they liked reading more, and wished that they were better readers. They also felt that they would be better readers if they read more. Those who had 131 children said that they wished their children would like reading more and would be better readers than they. . When asked if they expected their reading patterns to change in the future, almost all said that they would like to read more. "Would they read more for pleasure?" I asked. Those who had enjoyed reading a few novels or non- fiction books said that they would like to find more inter- esting books to read or to read more for enjoyment. Howev- er, most said that they didn’t expect to engage in pleasure reading. Because they have learned to entertain themselves in other ways (through sports or television watching), starting recreational reading seemed alien to them. Al- though they might wish they read more or they might like reading more, the kind of reading they could imagine doing more of was the kind of reading they were used to--informa- tional reading. CHAPTER 6 ANALYSIS OF RESULTS This chapter will consist of two parts. The first part will answer the question, "Does aliteracy exist among community college students?" The answer will be based on the findings of the closed-ended portion of the survey. In the second part, I will attempt to answer the question, "What are the extent and the nature of the aliteracy among Kalamazoo Valley Community College students?" using the results of both the closed- and open-ended portions of the survey. The findings of the interviews, which I summarized in Chapter 5, will be incorporated in the discussion. Aliteracy in American Society In Chapter 3, Review of the Literature, I offered answers to the research question, "To what extent are claims about the extent of aliteracy as a problem in this society accurate?" II reviewed reports on small- and large-scale surveys of the reading habits of many segments of the American population, from elementary-age children to adults. I based my conclusion about the extent of alitera- cy in the United States primarily on the reports on surveys of nationally representative samples of school-age and 132 133 adult populations. Among these are surveys of the Book Industry Study Group (BISG) and the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). I also used the findings of other major surveys, such as those by Sharon (1973-74), by Mikulecky et al. (1979) and by Anderson et al. (1988). I concluded from the findings of these surveys that there are bright and dark areas in the picture, borrowing a metaphor from Lehr (1985). One’s interpretation of the extent of aliteracy in this country would depend on where one’s eyes are focused. Those who have chosen to look at the bright areas have concluded, as Sharon has, that read- ing is a "ubiquitous activity" in American life. After all, 96 percent of the public read books, magazines, 9; newspapers (Boorstin, 1984), and the average person spends an hour and forty minutes reading on a typical day, with 76 minutes of that time devoted to non-job-related reading (Sharon, 1973-74). Also more than half (56 percent) of the adult population are book readers, according to the BISG surveys. Trends, especially concerning the reading attitudes and habits of students, represent the dark areas of the picture. For example, according to NAEP findings, children report that they like reading less as they grow older and that almost half of the 17-year—olds chose reading a book as their least favorite form of recreation. And the study by Anderson, et al. revealed that the typical American fifth grader reads "maybe as little as 8-12 minutes per day 134 when all types of reading material are counted and maybe as little as 4-5 minutes a day when only books are counted" (p. 299). If one were to focus on these trends, one has reason for saying that aliteracy is a problem in this society. Likewise, one could focus on the almost half of the adult population who are not book readers, according to the BISG surveys, and conclude that, because, compared to book reading, non-book reading tends to be accompanied by a lower amount of reading, more negative attitude toward reading, fewer types of texts read, fewer reasons for reading, lower intensity of motivations for reading, and poorer reading ability, this large a percentage represents a serious problem. There are murky areas in the picture as well. In separate investigations of whether or not reading activity has declined over several decades, Mikulecky et al. (1979) and Robinson (1980) have drawn contradictory conclusions. Mikulecky et al. report "an increase in adults’ total reading time" (p. 32). In his analysis of daily reading habits of Americans, Robinson, on the other hand, concludes that reading appears "to be a less prominent feature of daily life for all ages" (p. 141). In brief, aliteracy is or isn"t a problem depending on which studies one examines. 135 Aligeracy among Kelamezoo Valley Community College Students To ascertain if aliteracy exists among community college students, I will compare the results of a portion of the present survey to the survey findings cited above. In answering whether aliteracy exists in the American adult population, I had primarily used figures that relate to reading habits and to types of texts read because the reports of nationally representative surveys have no fig- ures available on the other components of aliteracy. Consequently, I will use the findings in this study rela- tive to those two components in determining whether alit- eracy exists among KVCC students. Reading Habits The results of the KVCC survey show that KVCC stu- dents’ reading habits are similar to those of the American adult population. In the BISG survey 55 percent had read at least one book in the last six months (they were re- ferred to as book readers). In the KVCC survey, 53.2 percent read at least one book not required for courses in the past month, and 54.3 percent had completed reading at least one book within the last six months (these two very similar figures represent the "book readers" in this.sam- ple). In the BISG survey, 39 percent read only magazines or newspapers in the past six months; in the present survey 45.4 percent read only magazines or newspapers in the past 136 Percentages 60 560% I 1983 BISG I chc Book Readers of Non- Readers Newspapers Readers and/or Magazines Only 1983 BISG = based on the numbers of respondents who reported reading books, newspapers and/or magazines only, or none of these materials in the previous six monthe. KVCC = based on the numbers of respondents who reported reading books, newspapers and/or maga— zines only, or none of these materials in the preyiogs heath. (N=219) Figure 1 Comparison of BISG and KVCC Results 137 month. The 1983 BISG survey showed that 96 percent of American adults were readers of books, magazines er news- papers in the last six months; the present survey shows 98.6 percent of the students surveyed are readers of books, magazines, er newspapers in the past month. Hence, in the BISG survey, 4 percent were non-readers of books, maga— zines, e; newspapers, compared to the 1.4 percent who are non-readers of these materials in the present survey. Figure 1 shows a comparison of the results of the 1983 BISG survey and of the present survey. While the number of readers of books, magazines, or newspapers among KVCC students seems very high, it must be noted that a portion of these readers are occasional read- ers rather than regular readers. The numbers are lower when we count only those who read magazines and newspapers regularly (on a daily or weekly basis). In this survey, 62.1 percent say they read magazines regularly, compared with 88 percent who say they have read magazines in the past month. Similarly, 82 percent say that they read newspapers regularly compared with 92 percent who say that they have read newspapers in the past month. Non—Book Reading Although the percentage of non-readers of books, magazines, or newspapers in the K.V.C.C. sample is a mere 1.4 percent, the percentage of non-book readers is 45.4 percent. As shown in the literature review, non-book 138 reading is important indicator of aliteracy in that it usually implies the following characteristics: lower amount_ of reading, more negative attitude toward reading, fewer types of texts read, fewer reasons for reading, lower intensity of motivations for reading, and lower reading ability. We can expect, therefore, that 45.4 percent (slightly less than half) of the K.V.C.C. sample, who indicated that they were non-book readers, would tend to have the characteristics listed above. Figures 2 through 6 show that this expectation is correct. Figure 2 shows that book readers in the sample tended to have more positive attitudes toward reading than non- book readers. Figure 3 shows that book readers tended to read more types of texts (books, magazines, and newspapers) than non-book readers. We see in Figure 4 that book read- ers tended to have a greater variety of motivations for reading than non-book readers, and in Figure 5 that book readers tended to have a higher intensity of motivations for reading than non-book readers. Figure 6 shows that book readers tended to View themselves as better readers than non-book readers. The Netupe ef Alitemacy Among KVCQ Smggemts In the following section, I will discuss important findings from both the survey and the interviews concerning the nature of aliteracy among KVCC students. 139 Percentages 8O ‘650% I Book Readers Non-Book Readers High Medium Low Favorablllty’ of Attitude Book readers are those who said they read a book all the way through in the previous six months. High attitude means a scale score of 11-15; medium attitude means a scale score of 6-10; low attitude means a scale score of 0—5. The difference between book readers and non-book readers produced a Chi square with p<.01. (K.V.C.C. sample, n=219) Figure 2 Attitude toward Reading 140 Percentages so ' 64.9% so - I BookReaders 40 _ 37.8% I Non-Book Readers 20- High Medium Low Number of Types Book readers are those who said they read a book all the way through in the previous six months. High number of types means a scale score of 11—15; medium number of types means a scale score of 6-10; low number of types means a scale score of 0-5. The difference between book readers and non-book readers produced a Chi Square with p<.01. (K.V.C.C. sample, n—219) Figure 3 Types of Texts Read 141 Percentages 60 53.7% . Book Readers Non-Book Readers High Medium Low Number of Motivations Book readers are those who said they read a book all the way through in the previous six months. High variety means a scale score of 4-5; medium variety means a scale score of 2-3; low variety means a scale score of 0-1. The difference between book readers and non-book readers produced a Chi Square with p<.01. (K.V.C.C. sample, n-219) Figure 4 Variety of Motivations 142 Percentages 80 62.1% I Book Readers Non-Book Readers High Medium Low intensity Book readers are those who said they read a book all the way through in the previous six months. High intensity means a scale score of 19-25; medium intensity means a scale score of 12-18; low intensity means a scale score of 5-11. The difference between book readers and non-book readers produced a Chi Square with p<.01. (K.V.C.C. sample, n=219) Figure 5 Intensity of Motivations T———( 143 Percentages 100 80" I Book Readers 60- Non-Book Readers 40‘ 20" 4.2% 3_1% High Medium Low Ability Book readers are those who said they read a book all the way through in the previous six months. High ability means a scale score of 12—15; medium ability means a scale score of 7-11; low ability means a scale score of 3-6. The difference between book readers and non—book readers produced a Chi Square with p<.01. (K.V.C.C. sample, n=219) Figure 6 Self—Perceived Reading Ability T_____f 144 In the essay portion of the questionnaire, the stu- dents were asked to respond in writing to one question: "Why do you think you are the kind of reader that you are today? What or who might have influenced your reading attitudes and habits? For example, can you recall past experiences, whether they occurred at home or at school, that might explain why you feel the way you do about read- ing?" Of the 219 participants in the study, 10 (4.6 per- cent) did not answer the essay question. Those who did wrote answers that range in length from a few words or a phrase to several sentences. students’ answers to the essay question in the questionnaire will be summarized and incor- porated in the following discussion of significant findings that have emerged from the total data. Student’s Perceptione of the Inflmemees on Their Reading Although this study is not primarily concerned with identifying the reasons why students read or don’t read, the essay question that dealt with that subject (question 14 of the questionnaire, a copy of which is in the Appen- dix) produced interesting and potentially significant findings. Not all the students answered the question, and not all the answers were pertinent to the question asked. Neither can the answers be taken as complete explanations. The following discussion focuses on the students’ percep- tions of what or who had influenced them as readers. 145 The "reasons" or "influences" cited by the students can be categorized as extrinsic or environmental and in- trinsic or personal. Home- and school-related influences fall under extrinsic or environmental. Reading skills and personal traits, such as a sense of curiosity or having learned to read early, belong to the intrinsic or personal category. These categories are used purely for descriptive purposes. By grouping the influences in this manner, I do not intend to imply that they are mutually exclusive; evidence points to an interaction among the different types of influences. In other words, a person might have learned to read early because of parents who read to him and pro- vided him with books to read. Some responses written by the students list just one type of influence while others list more than one. More often than not, a combination of reasons or influences are implied by the comments. Whenev- er I cite students’ responses in the discussion that fol- lows, I cite them in full, so that the reader may see the complete response given to the question. Table 1 presents extrinsic and intrinsic reasons, showing that extrinsic reasons were cited five times more often than intrinsic reasons. The students’ comments may also be categorized into positive and negative comments (see Table 2). The vast majority of comments are positive, attesting to the will- ingness of those who have favorable attitudes toward read- ing and/or who read a considerable amount to share their 146 Table 1 Reasons for Reading Parent(s) 84 Teachers and teaching methods 36 Grandparents and other relatives 10 Friends 9 Boring textbooks 9 Lack of interesting reading materials 3 Liereriaps 2 Total 153 Intpinsic or Personal Reasons Problems or difficulties with reading 18 Learning to read at an early age 5 Preference for sports and active pasttimes 4 3 Pmeference for television and movies Total 30 views about why they liked to read or why they read as much as they did. The majority of the positive comments concern the home environment and the contributions of parents and other family members toward the formation of an interest in reading. Generally, when one or both parents are mentioned as a positive influence, at least one other factor is mentioned as woll--for example, the availability of books or an encouraging teacher. Home Environment Almost all of the written answers to the essay ques- tion (question 14) show a combination of factors that encourage a person to like to read and to become a reader. For example, in addition to regularly reading to the child, a parent might also buy books for her or take her to the 147 Table 2 Positive and Negative Influences on Reading Behavior Poeitive Influences Timee Cited Parent(s) 65 Teachers and teaching methods 26 Grandparents and other relatives 16 Friends 10 Reading a lot as a child 9 Learning to read at an early age 5 Librarians ' 2 Total 133 Negative Influences Lack of parental support 19 Personal problems or difficulties with reading ' (e.g., dyslexia, poor concentration and concentration, slow reading, poor vision) 18 Poor teachers and teaching methods 10 Boring and ineffectual textbooks and reading materials Not reading as a child Preference for sports and active pasttimes Preference for television and movies Lack of interesting reading materials Parentsgpushinglreading, Total 148, library. Of course, parents are more likely to do this if they themselves are readers. Seeing their parents read has encouraged these students to read as well. For some students, encouragement has also come from friends and from extended family members, like aunts and grandparents. Their comments suggest that these readers lived in an environment that supported reading. I’m the kind of reader that I am today because of the encouragement I received as a child. I can remember being read to by my mom, seeing her read, and of course going to buy books. I also remember fondly one reading teach- er whom I really loved. The teachers in middle school who read aloud to classes always im— pressed me. *** I think one of the reasons I enjoy reading is because I grew up being read to. My parents read books a lot to me, my teachers, and other relatives. I also saw my mom read almost every night and she still does! She taught me that reading relaxes you and can be very enjoyable! *** My parents valued the ability to read. When I was young, regardless of how badly I had be- haved, my mother could always find an excuse to buy a book for me when I wanted one and encour- aged me to read for school assignments and per- sonal enjoyment. *** I think I was influenced by my parents. Since I was very young my mother spent a lot of time reading to me. As I grew older they would buy me books and take me to the library and help me find the type of book I was interested in. I was always encouraged and sometimes rewarded for finishing a book. 149 Like some of the students I interviewed who cited lack of parental support, some respondents described homes in which reading was not encouraged. I wasn't pushed as a child to develop my interest in reading. It was always easier to turn on the television. My parents rarely read to us and there wasn’t much of a reading selec- tion in the house while I was growing up. *** I do not like to read very much. I am trying to read a book right now for pleasure. I think I missed that in my childhood. My mother hates to read and my father never does. I want to read more because I am lacking in that area. I like to read once I get started. I always feel I don’t have the time. Likewise, as in the interviews, mothers who read a lot and father who didn’t were cited. In the cases below, the father's influence prevailed: I am an average reader. My mother reads a lot, my father doesn’t read much. I don’t either because I do too many things. *** Books just never interested me. I always found something else to do: Playing sports and some other kind of activity. I’m not sure who influenced me the way I’m about reading today. Possibly my father because he couldn’t read very well, and at a young age everyone likes to be like their father. I’ve just always read what I had to get by. But now I find it more important to read. Compared to the number of references to home environ- ment as a source of influence on reading interests and habits, the number of references to not following parental or home influence is small (about 10 citations compared to over a hundred citations on both the positive and negative 150 influences of the home on the students’ reading interests and habits). The following student wrote that even though her parents were not readers, she became interested in reading. However, she didn’t say how she became an avid reader. I enjoy reading for relaxation. Most of the time reading text books is not very relaxing. Once I start reading a novel I have a hard time putting it down. My family has never been the type that everyone reads books-—in fact I’ve never seen my parents pick up a book but both are very successful people with no time to read. So I guess I’m the black sheep. They never stressed the importance of reading. I just do it for enjoyment. On the other hand, although the following respondent had parents and friends who read, she didn’t develop a "passion" for reading: I’m not sure why my feelings for reading are so poor. My mother and father read and many friends also do. I just do not have a passion for reading. My reading skills are good, like I said there just isn’t a passion there for read- ing. A few responses suggest that some parents might have used methods that discouraged reading. For instance, the parents might have "pushed" reading too hard so that a child rebelled or might have forced on children materials they were not interested in. This respondent cited her speech impediment combined with being forced to read aloud to her mother as the reasons why she hated to read. Maybe because when I was young, I was forced to read aloud to my mom. I always hated it, and I was slow in reading and pronouncing the words. I had to have 3 years of speech class to learn 151 how to speak, and I’ve always felt self conscious of the way I talk. [This person indicated in other parts of the questionnaire that she didn’t like to read and never read for enjoyment.] School Environment Next to parental or home influence, schooling was cited as a second major source of influence. Respondents cited the positive influences of teachers and schooling both generally and specifically. An example of a general, rather vague, comment is "My teachers back in grade school made us read a lot, so I think it carried over all the years of my years in school." Then there are comments that state exactly what a teacher did. In the following in— stance, the respondent was influenced by specific books that his teacher read to his class: The books that my fourth grade teacher read to my class inspired me to read the same type of book. Most of the time she would read the first book in the series, i.e., "tales of the 4th grade nothing, 7 chronicles of Narnia, the house with a clock in its walls." The following respondent wrote that having been given the freedom to choose books she could read increased her inter- est in reading. I think I’ve always liked to read but I remember that in a fifth of sixth grade class we were allowed to choose the books we wanted to read and I was very excited about it. I liked most novels that we were required to read [for] Classes but not as much as the ones I chose. Some students wrote comments that reveal disappoint- ment or disenchantment with their educational experience both in general and specific ways. 152 I believe my reading habits were influenced by my education through high school. I disliked the educational process and desired to learn what was of interest to me through other channels (i.e. personal reading). *** I believe if my past English teacher would have taken the time to show the enjoyment of reading instead of showing the job it takes to get reading done I might be a better reader. The following respondents identified themselves as "poor readers" in school who suffered from such school practices as constant skills testing and lack of free choice in reading: I used to be a very poor reader. We were always tested for skills in grammar in junior high school. I felt very pressured and could not read fast enough. Consequently, I disliked reading and never did so on my own. After I married, I began reading a few novels for entertainment while my husband was out of town. I really began to enjoy it when I could do so at my own pace. Now I read all the time and love it. Plus my speed has greatly improved. *** I was a poor reader in elementary school--so I hated reading. I was forced to read certain books for school. I did not enjoy reading until I got out of high school, when I had a lot of time on my hands and found material that inter- ested me. Interestingly, for many students, reading interest was sparked by an opportunity to read what they wanted.'With the free reading came enjoyment because they found material they were interested in. Many respondents who did not enjoy reading while they where reading for pleasure was not were in school, 153 stressed, learned to enjoy it at a later age. Having discovered leisure reading on their own, they are able to separate what they didn’t enjoy from what they did. Re- spondents who enjoyed reading pointed out the difference between school-required reading and self-chosen reading: I’m not especially fond of reading textbooks because they are boring a lot of times. However, I do read them and try to understand what I’ve read. If only started reading for pleasure two years ago. It is enjoyable and relaxing. When I was in elementary school, reading for fun was not encouraged. *** Probably because we as students are always required to read textbooks throughout a class and that’s not much fun. Reading what you want to Egad is a lot more fun than reading what you neye On the other hand, students who didn’t enjoy reading appeared to consider reading as a monolithic activity and were unable to distinguish pypee of reading. For instance, some of the students I interviewed made blanket statements about reading such as "I don’t like to read" or "I don’t enjoy reading." Only when given the chance to talk about what they liked or didn’t like were they able to identify the materials they did enjoy reading, such as magazine articles. Perhaps, as Weaver (1989) and Neilson (1989, p. 70) have suggested, students have internalized the definition of reading implied by their classes. They have learned to define "reading" in terms of school reading, the reading of textbooks and other school materials. As one of the 154 persons I interviewed (Tom) explained, he answered the questionnaire the way he did (saying that he didn’t like to read and never read for his own enjoyment) because "most of the stuff that I read or that I’m told to read is school stuff and I don’t like to read it." In the following written comment, a respondent explains her negative atti- tude toward reading in terms of what she did for school. I never really wanted to read. For classes I would read the questions the teacher gave us, skim and find the answer. I would never obtain any information from the reading. Perhaps, given the chance to explain this answer, the student would be able to say more about reading she did enjoy. Nevertheless, from the answers of those "lucky souls" who were able to discover pleasure reading largely on their own, educators, such as Chambers (1969), Fader (1976), and Smith (1983, 1986), are justified in their criticisms of the schools. Such criticisms, as explained in Chapter 2, are directed against the overuse of text— books, the overemphasis on utilitarian reading and the corresponding neglect of pleasure reading, the forcing of "good books" on students, and the constant testing for discrete skills. Reading Aptitudes Like the students surveyed by the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP, 1981), KVCC students believe 155 that reading is important in the abstract. Reading is highly rated by the students: 94 percent agreed or strongly agreed with the statement that "reading is a useful activi- ty." However, asked to respond to the statement "I like to read," only 80.4 percent agree/strongly agreed with the statement, 14.1 percent disagree/strongly disagreed, and 4.1 percent had no opinion. When asked how many times they read for enjoyment in their spare time, 13.7 percent said "never," and 21 percent said "less than once a week." As Table 3 shows, "liking to read" is significantly associated with "reading for enjoyment." Those who report— ed that they liked to read also tended to read more fre- quently than those who say they didn’t like to read. However, liking to read is not synonymous with choos- ing to read for enjoyment. Some students answered that Table 3 Liking to Read vs. Reading for Enjoyment "I Like to Read" Reading for Disagree or Strongly Agree Enjoyment Neutral or Agree Total Less than once a 34 42 76 week 79.1% 23.9% 34.7% Once a week or 9 134 143 more 20.9% 76.1% 65.3% Total 43 176 N=219 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 156 they never read for enjoyment or read for enjoyment less than once a week even though they reported that they liked to read. They represent 19.2 percent of the sample. Considering that the respondents are students, one can say that the amount of reading required of students might have affected the amount of recreational reading that they engaged in. Gende; as a Predictor of Aliterecy Many studies have found gender to be a strong predic- tor of reading habits and attitudes. That finding is confirmed by the present study. As Figure 7 shows, females reported reading more books than males: 67.7 percent of the females said they had read a book, wholly or partly, in the past month, compared to only 39 percent of the males. However, slightly more males than females regularly read magazines, and slightly more males than females regularly read newspapers. Further, females reported more positive attitudes toward reading than males, as Figure 8 shows. Mothers appear to be more influential than fathers on students’ reading attitudes and habits, judging from the answers to the essay question. Mothers were cited as a positive influence 22 times and as a negative influence 2 157 Percentages um 860% I Male I Female Book Magazine Newspaper Readers Readers Readers The difference between males and females for book reading produced a Chi Square with p<.01. The differences between males and females for magazine and newspaper reading were not statistically significant. (K.V.C.C. sample, n=219) Figure 7 Gender and Reading Behavior Percentages 60— 50 40 I Male I)! I F emaie 20- High Medium Low Favorabilitg of Attitude The difference between males and females produced a Chi Square with p<.01. (K.V.C.C. sample, n=219) Figure 8 Gender and Attitude Toward Reading 159 times. Fathers, on the other hand, were cited only 6 times as a positive influence and 5 times as a negative influ- ence. Having had mothers read to them was an often cited reason for students becoming interested in reading. Occasionally, students mentioned that their mothers read a lot, but their fathers read little, if any. Among the 9 interview subjects, 5 stated that their mothers were avid readers, some of whom tried to interest their sons and daughters in the reading of fiction. Fa- thers were mentioned 3 times mainly as readers of news- papers and magazines. These findings are consistent with the figures from the survey showing that females read more books, and males read slightly more newspapers and maga- zines. Reasons for Reading Among the indicators of aliteracy discussed in Chap- ter 3 is the tendency to read for only one reason rather than for several purposes. Students who are avid readers show an awareness of different purposes for reading. Note, for instance, the variety of purposes for reading and types of reading material (fiction and non-fiction) implied in the following written responses from obviously avid read- ers: As a young child I always read a lot of books. I had a wild imagination and needed to read to satisfy this. My Grandmother encouraged me. She would.always buy me new books, new 160 stories. When I got older, I still had the need or drive to read. I need a variety of books, a need to learn about lots of different things or different areas of knowledge. *** I enjoy reading. I like to know what’s going on. I read for pleasure and educational purposes. My parents, high school teachers, influenced my reading habits. Students who didn’t like to read tended to be less aware that there is more than one type of reading or more than one purpose for reading. They tended to make blanket statements like "I don’t like to read." or "I never read for enjoyment." However, when I asked some of the in- terviewees what they did read, they mentioned materials, such as articles relating to their hobbies and interests, which they said they enjoyed reading. Students who report- ed that they didn’t like to read and that they avoided reading appeared less capable of making distinctions be- tween types of reading. Among the students I interviewed, reading for informa- tion seemed to be the most prevalent, if not the sole, reason for reading. The said that they read primarily for school, and because school reading stresses reading for information, their reason for reading is usually to get information. However, most of them read magazine articles on their own, and these students said that they enjoyed reading these articles. Perhaps they viewed magazine reading as purely reading for information and did not 161 consider the pleasurable elements in the reading experi- ence. Again, because they perceived themselves as individ- uals who did not like reading, they were not able to see the distinctions among reading experiences. Inn contrast, a respondent, who wrote that he liked to read but preferred informational materials, also wrote that reading for infor- mation and reading for pleasure could overlap. His answer implies he was aware that there are different reasons for reading and that he realized that because he derived pleas- ure from reading informative materials, the distinctions commonly made between reading for pleasure and reading for information are artificial. (Reading theorists would describe his awareness and ability as metacognition [Weav- er, 1988, p. 23].) Some of the interviewees, like Sue, did not see a place for recreational reading in their future. Sue said that she would read a book if it happened to be the only option available to her, such as if she were put in jail, "but it wouldn’t be my first choice." She would not read for pleasure because she preferred other ways of entertain- ing herself. Still, she wished that she read more, but more of the same type of reading, informational or utili- tarian reading. But if she disliked this kind of reading and had developed a habit of avoiding it, what is the likelihood that she would read more? It seems that the prospects for change are nil for these students. Perhaps some will develop new interests, as Keith did, when he IF" 162 became vice president of his father’s company and started to read articles on management. And perhaps some will continue to read articles in magazines and/or newspapers on tOpics that interest them end continue to avoid books. Some of the interview subjects had experienced pleas- ure reading a few times and spoke about their reactions to these experiences. Three who could recall one book that they enjoyed reading as adults expressed surprise at how interested and involved they were in their reading. They indicated that they didn’t experience any of the problems they usually had when they read textbooks. Among these problems were not keeping their minds on what they are reading and not understanding new words. Most of those who had had pleasurable experiences with books expressed the wish that they could find more interesting books to read. Some respondents wrote comments indicating that they had learned to read for pleasure on their own. As one student wrote, "High school and college have forced me into the habit of reading what I have to. I’m still working on improving my reading for fun or just for pleasure." Com- ments such as this show that a pattern of reading only what one has to or reading only for school can be broken. However, for those who have a strong perception that they do not like to read, the pattern may be harder to break. W—E *5“ Typee ef Texpe Read Some users of the word "aliterate" have used it to refer to non-book readers. Non-book reading by itself cannot be equated with aliteracy because to do so is to deny the importance of other types of text and to minimize the role of other components, such as attitude. Some people who like to read may not be book readers. For example, 5 students who answered the essay question cited a preference for magazines and/or newspapers, indicating that they liked to read, but they liked to read magazines and/or newspapers, not books. The following student wrote that he liked to read, but his reading difficulties had caused him to limit his current reading to magazines: As I was growing up I was a slow reader and hated to read, especially out loud during class. I took reading classes that did help. In college I read books (text) every day and the more I read the faster I get. Reading takes practice. Maybe someday I’ll enjoy reading books but now I’ll stick to magazines. In Chapter 5, I stated that many of the interviewees who reported that they didn’t like to read and that they never or rarely engaged in pleasure reading based those answers on their feelings about book reading. In the sur- vey, 30 students (13.7 percent of the sample) said that they never read for enjoyment; of the 30, 12 students (36.7 percent of those who never read for enjoyment) indicated that they regularly read both magazines and newspapers. Only 2 students (6.7 percent) of those who never read for 164 enjoyment did not read magazines em newspapers. Why didn’t some of these students consider magazine or newspaper reading as reading for enjoyment? In interviewing 9 of these 30 students, I learned that 7 of the 9 had book reading in mind when they answered the question. In fact, they did read newspapers and/or magazines regularly. However, they disliked book reading, a dislike which colored their perception of reading in general. On the basis of these finding, one might infer that some students read-more than they thought and that they liked to read more than they thought. Reeding Ability As I explained in Chapter 6, reading ability has generally not received enough attention as a component of aliteracy. It has been assumed that aliterates are people who are not hampered by poor reading skills. This percep- tion partly comes from the common definition of aliterame as a person who has the ability to read but doesn’t or won’t read and the dichotomous meaning given to "ability to read" (i.e., a person either gen read or cannem). There- fore, since almost all people "can read," it follows that those who don’t read much don’t read because they prefer not to. Little attention has been given to the role of reading ability in a person’s lack of desire to read. Many studies have shown that a positive relationship exists 165 between reading ability (as measured by standardized tests) and reading volume; the more peeple read, the higher their scores in achievement tests. Book readers, who tend to read more than non-book readers, generally receive higher test scores than non-book readers. In the present study, students’ reading ability was self-rated rather than measured by tests. Students rated themselves as readers .on a continuum from "poor" to "excellent"; they rated their reading comprehension and speed in the same way. The findings of this survey support other surveys that have found that reading ability and reading volume go hand in hand. In this study, as Figure 6 shows, those who read more tend to regard themselves as better readers than those who don’t read as much. Reading difficulties constitute a major group of intrinsic reasons the students gave for not reading. Of those who answered the essay question, 9 percent wrote that they experienced reading difficulties such as poor vision, dyslexia, Attention Deficit Disorder, slow speed, poor comprehension, and limited vocabulary. Roughly a third of these students report that despite their difficulties they like reading. Some wrote that they tried to read more to overcome their reading problems. About two-thirds of those who cited reading difficulties indicated that their difficulties caused them to dislike or avoid reading. The student who wrote the following comment said that he liked 166 to read despite his reading difficulties. waever, obvi- ously his reading difficulties limited or hampered his reading. I like to read but because I don’t read very well, it makes it hard to keep reading or even to get the reading done. The problem comes from grade school. I never learned to read very well or spell. Among the students I interviewed, four considered themselves below average or poor readers, four thought of themselves as average readers, and one indicated he was an above average reader. However, most of the students I interviewed, including some who considered themselves “average," mentioned a problem or difficulty with reading. They described such difficulties as poor word recognition, difficulty in keeping their minds on what they are reading, and slow rate of reading. These students said that their difficulties resulted from lack of reading and/or caused them to find reading unpleasant and to read less. As Winkle (1988) explains, while poor reading ability may not by itself cause aliteracy, it may discourage a person from reading. The less a person reads, the less likely he will develop his skills. Most of the students I interviewed seemed to realize this. They thought that their dislike of reading had negatively affected their reading ability and their school work. The National Acade- my of Education Commission on Reading (1985) says, Predictably poor readers have unfavorable atti- tudes toward reading. What is not so predictable is whether lack of proficiency in reading stems 167 from unfavorable attitudes or whether it is the other way around. Probably the truth can lie in either direction. (p. 15) To understand a person’s aliteracy, therefore, it is important to consider the role that reading ability plays in his or her reading. Although by itself it cannot/6e considered a cause of aliteracy, poor reading ability can start the cycle of disliking reading and avoiding it, leading to further deterioration of reading skills. It is important to note that most of the interviewees' difficulties applied to textbook reading. Some stated that they didn’t have as much difficulty when they read material that interested them. One of these students, for instance, had been reading pamphlets and books about real estate because he wanted to be a realtor. He said that he had no problem reading these materials because he had some idea what they were about. In other words, some of these diffi- culties might have resulted from having to read material that was poorly written (as many textbooks are) and materi- al that is unfamiliar and uninteresting to them. Every reader, regardless of how fluent, competent, or experi- enced, has experienced similar difficulties when reading material that is not well written or material with which he or she is not familiar or knowledgeable. We have reason to conclude that some of the problems described by these students might have resulted from school practices such as the use of uninteresting, poorly written textbooks. Fur- thermore, they might have been caused by simply lack of 168 reading-~resulting in limited exposure to certain types of reading material and in insufficient background knowledge and schemata to understand these materials (Weaver, 1988, p. 17). The real danger may lie in the attitudes and beliefs that students who are daunted and frustrated by school reading acquire about reading and about themselves as readers. The students I interviewed considered themselves people who didn't like reading. The more they didn’t like to read, the more they avoided reading. Although most of them actually read more than they thought (and perhaps liked reading more than they thought), their reading was limited and they felt inadequate as readers and as stu- dents. Multi-dimensional Natume of Aliteracy Intrinsic reasons, such as a personal preference for learning by doing rather than by reading and other idio- syncrasies, are often cited with extrinsic reasons for not reading. For example, the following students wrote that their inclination to be active (intrinsic) may be the result of their "lifestyle" or home environment (extrinsic). I don’t care to read. For a lot of times I think it’s boring. Reading makes me tired. I think my lifestyle has a lot to do with it, because I like to always be active. I learn better by doing things than by reading. 169 *** Reading was never a big interest in my life. When I was growing up I enjoyed being with my family and friends most of the time. When I was reading I was alone and small things would dis— tract me. I always wanted to "do" things not just sit around reading. Reading was boring to me. Maybe I never found anything to read that interested me. In most of these responses, a number of "reasons" or "influences" are cited, and it’s difficult, if not impossi- ble, to separate them. When different "reasons" are cited, I believe it is reasonable to assume that they are to be interpreted as multiple, not as single, isolated reasons. For instance, Sue, a student I interviewed, said that she] thought that the biggest influence on her reading attitudes; and habits was that she was not encouraged to read on her. own at home or at school when she was growing up. Howev- er, she also said, "I enjoy doing things with my hands or one-on-one than I have reading about things, so when it came to reading I didn’t comprehend as well." These influ- ences--lack of encouragement and modeling of reading, preference for learning by doing rather than by reading, and poor comprehension--seem inextricably linked. It is impossible to separate them and to say which had a stronger effect. Sometimes contradictory answers are given, making the analysis even more complicated. Hence, additional informa- tion offered in other parts of the questionnaire should be considered. It is only when we take into account all the 170 information available that we can have confidence that the picture or profile we are trying to reconstruct is an accurate one. Any part of the questionnaire responses interpreted by itself may lead to inaccurate conclusions. Furthermore, the questionnaire answers often do not provide a complete or clear picture. The following response com- bined with other information taken from the questionnaire, illustrates the different components of aliteracy interact- ing in a person’s life: 'When I was little I really didn’t read very much so I never got in the habit to read very fast. Also when I read I can’t comprehend what I have read because I am easily distracted. My parents might have influenced my reading habits. I guess the reason I don’t like reading is be- cause my teachers at school would give me home- work to read and it would be about 20 or more pages. Plus I would have other homework on top of that. I just couldn’t comprehend all of the reading so I thought it was a waste of time. Here we can see meeglng_neneylem (reading little when young), reading ability (slow speed and poor comprehension), and emmlmnde_me_meeding (negative--"it was waste of time") interacting to produce a configuration that might make us conclude the person is "aliterate." We might expect this student to dislike reading and to avoid read- ing, especially book reading. However, the picture isn’t that simple. Her other answers in the questionnaire said that she liked to read (this seems to contradict what she wrote in the paragraph above) and that she had read a book in the past six months (she is a "book reader," in this study). She also said she read two newspapers regularly. 171 However, she rated herself below average as a reader; she rated her comprehension also as below average and her speed of reading as poor. Despite her difficulties with school reading, she said she read for enjoyment one-to-two times a week. The picture or profile of this student resists categorization. It is easier to say that it is unique to this individual. Aliteracy is a result of the interactions of the different components. We can compare readers and point out similarities and differences, but at the end we have to acknowledge that each reader is unique. The push and pull of any number of elements can result in a reading configu- ration that probably only that particular reader has. Hence, Taylor’s comment (1990, p. 4) that a person’s literacy may be as unique as his or her fingerprints ap- pears to be Valid. Finally, the answer to the question "Is this person aliterate?" is not a simple one. Assessment of a person’s literacy or aliteracy entails looking at different compo— nents, judging the degree to which each component operates, and analyzing the various ways in which they interact and influence each other. It entails familiarity with people’s lives--knowing the person well enough that one’s interpre- tation of their answers is based on an adequate under- standing of their experiences and perspectives on reading. 172 It is possible to determine the extent of a person's alit- eracy, but we must explain and qualify our answer. CHAPTER 7 CONCLUSIONS AND SPECULATIONS Much of the literature on aliteracy and reading reluc- tance has focused on giving practical advice and recommen- dations on how to alleviate aliteracy among young people. Additionally, most educators and social commentators who have written about aliteracy have relied largely on common knowledge or anecdotal material about aliteracy. Without defining aliteracy or explaining with some precision what they mean by it, they have made pronouncements and recom- mendations about the "problem" of aliteracy. Furthermore, many have bemoaned the "decline" of reading, and the "sWelling ranks of aliterates," without offering conclu- sive evidence that aliteracy (whatever it is) does exist and to what extent it does. In this study, I have attempted to answer important questions about aliteracy, using theoretical and empirical methods. Partly because the term is too new to be in the dictionary, there was a need to define "aliteracy" precise- ly. To know how aliteracy should be defined, I broadened the scope of the study by reviewing the literature on the history of literacy and literacy instruction. I derived an operational definition of aliteracy from the literature on aliteracy and related topics. I also conducted a survey 173 174 of community college students using questionnaires, and I interviewed nine students who showed aliterate characteris- tics. My goals in this chapter are to discuss the meanings and implications of the findings presented in Chapter 6 and to answer the research question, "In the light of new, comprehensive definitions of literacy, what are the impli- cations of aliteracy?" To accomplish these goals, I will compare popular beliefs and assumptions about aliteracy with the findings, conclusions and implications that have emerged from the review of the literature on literacy and aliteracy and from the survey and interviews. I will conclude by recommending some approaches that educators can take to reduce aliteracy and by suggesting some topics for future investigation. Eopular Beliefs abont Aliteracy yensus Einginge ef the may Those who complain about aliteracy generally represent the views of readers or people who have vested interests in reading--they are in publishing or in education. They usually cite the "decline" of reading activity or the "rise" of aliteracy in society, implying that there was a time in this country’s history when people read more, wrote more, knew more and participated enthusiastically in liter- ate activities. Tchudi (1980) explains that no "golden age" of literacy ever existed. Rather, as I have shown in 175 Chapter 2, a look at the history of literacy has revealed that definitions of literacy have changed and eXpectations for literacy have increased since the 18005. Further, it is not unusual to encounter the remark that aliterates abound at all levels of society. For in— stance, in the aliteracy conference sponsored by the Ameri- can Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research, Wilson referred to the "multitudes of people who read nothing. They do not buy newspapers, they do not read newspapers, they do not buy books, they do not read books. They are reading nothing . . . ." (Thimmesch, 1984, p. 18). Gener- alizations such as these need to be challenged. While it is true that there are people in this society who read nothing or almoep nothing, they represent a tiny fraction of the population--4 percent, according to the Book Indus- try Study Group survey (this survey measures only the reading of books, magazines, and newspapers). Researchers who have tried to measure how much time people spend read- ing have concluded that reading is a "ubiquitous activity" in American life (Sharon, 1973-74). They have found that people did read something-~if not books, then magazines, newspapers, documents, advertisements, signs, computer printouts, and other job-related materials. Sharon even concludes that the average adult spends an hour and 46 minutes reading in a typical day. Out of that time 76 minutes are spent on non—job-related reading. One might 176 conclude from these figures that complaints about aliteracy are unwarranted. As I have concluded in Chapter 3, the results of readership surveys are contradictory. The decision as to the extent of aliteracy depends in part on where one looks, at the bright spots or at the dark spots in the literacy picture. The fact that about 96 percent of American adults and about 98 percent of the students in the KVCC sample said that they read newspapers, magazines, 9; books is a bright spot. However, like the American adult pOpulation, almost half of the KVCC sample did not read books and tended to show aliterate characteristics. In brief, we need to base our conclusions on the extent of the problem of aliteracy, not just on quantifiable survey figures but also on a fuller understanding of literacy and aliteracy. We need, furthermore, to base our decisions on aliteracy on a thorough understanding of the lives and perspectives of the individuals whom we would label aliter- ate. Many educators and commentators lament the effects of television and see television as the villain in the "rise" of aliteracy. As Chapter 3-has shown, no significant relationship has been found between television watching and amount of time spent in reading. Likewise, the view that people who don’t read are simply too busy to read, implying that those who read are not busy, has not been borne out by research. Book readers tend to participate more in leisure and community activities than non-readers and non-book 177 readers. This heavier involvement in other activities has been found among both adult and school-age book readers. Misconceptions about literacy have influenced popular notions of aliteracy. Literacy experts now stress that literacy is a complex, multi-dimensional concept, not a dichotomy as our society has come to view it. Partly because of the vast number of people who are "literate" (popular meaning: have the basic ability to read), the number of those who appear unwilling to use their skill seems large. In effect, we have created another word to separate, this time those "who gen and de read" from those "who can but don’t or won’g" read. Just as the meaning of litenate has been simplified to a dichotomy, so has the meaning of aliterate. When we say that an aliterate can read, we imply that there is a standard separating those who can from those who cannot. And when we say, that an aliterate does not or will not read, we imply that there is a level of reading activity or willingness to read that we can all accept. What those levels are or should be no one has ascertained.' "Who deserves such a label?" and "Are we using it too loosely?" are good questions as well. As a result of the dichotomous meanings given to aliteracy, it has been assumed that aliterates have the ability to read, that their enly problem is that they don’t or won’t. In Chapter 3, I cited studies that proved that reading ability and reading volume or time spent in reading 178 are significantly related. This suggests that reading ability does play a more significant role in aliteracy than' we have assumed. The comprehensive definition of aliteracy used in this study includes six components: reading attitude, reading behavior, types of texts read, range of reasons for read- ing, intensity-of motivations for reading, and reading ability. These components interact with each other in multiple ways. Figure 9 shows these components as circles which overlap. When only two circles overlap or only two components are present, then aliteracy is slight. However, when all the components are present as shown in the middle, where all the circles touch, aliteracy is extreme. Each of these components is a continuum, interacting-with the others to form a configuration that may be highly individu- al. For example, a person who reads nothing but romance novels may be considered partly aliterate even though she reads voraciously. Why? In terms of the components of aliteracy, she reads only one type of material, she reads only for one type of reason, her motivation for reading may not be strong, and her reading ability may not be adequate for other types of reading. In her book Tne_meklng_ef_a_3eader (1984), Cochran- Smith stresses the complex and multi-dimensional nature of literacy. To study literacy, she suggests, a researcher has to examine "many layers of context"--"both a larger educational context and an almost limitless number of Figure 9 179 KVCC STUDENT POPULATION Negative Attitude Toward Reading Low Reading Ability Overlapping Components of Aliteracy 180 social contexts" (p. 256). It is important to recognize that although in the present study I have stressed the multiple components of aliteracy, what the survey and the interviews have revealed about students as readers is probably only a partial representation of what is there. There is a great deal more to the reading experience and to being a reader than the survey and interviews covered. For example, in terms of types of texts read, I only asked about the reading of books, newspapers, and magazines. I did not inquire about other materials read such as docu- ments related to jobs, hobbies, church, and other personal and community activities the students may participate in. I did not inquire about the actual time spent in reading or the number of pages read, although I did ask how often students read books, newspapers and magazines. I asked students how many types of texts—-books, magazines, news- papers and others—-they read, but I didn’t ask what types of books and what types of newspapers they read. I did not inquire about the nature of involvement in the reading, whether superficial (such as the scanning of a newspaper) or deep (such as the emotional, intellectual, analytical, and aesthetic involvement required in the reading of cer- tain fiction or non-fiction books). In other words, there is an almost infinite variety of reading experiences, which no study, no matter how in-depth, can fully explore. 181 Bleich (1988) says that literacy is as individual as religion, and Denny Taylor (1990) reasons that it is as unique as one’s fingerprints. One develops different lit eracies in different contexts, consonant with the roles one plays in society-~for example, as a voter, student, consum- er, member of the city planning committee; or according to one’s personal goals and needs--for example, as a moviego- er, television-viewer, hobbyist, or fiction-reader. In the pilot study I conducted prior to the main study, I talked to a female student who reported that she "hated to read." When asked if she read anything, she told me that she read the Bible regularly for a Bible—discussion class at her church. She actually read more than she revealed in the questionnaire, and the Bible is at least one text that she liked to read. Just as scholars today are arguing that there are multiple literacies, we can conclude that there are multiple aliteracies. Furthermore, just as literacy is a dynamic procese that never stands still and is impossible to capture in its totality, so is aliteracy. Cochran-Smith writes about the process of "documenting children in_the_nrooess of becoming literate: we can see their emerging and developing literacy as it occurs in everyday situations" (p. 256). The same process could be observed in the lives of the students as well, a process that did not reveal itself in the survey but in the interviews. During one of the interviews, Ann, a mother of a young child, talked about her desire to see 182 her child grow up to like reading more than she does. Because I am a children’s literature teacher, I told her that a course in children’s literature could expose her to variety of books she could read to her child. She said that she was interested in taking the course, and the next semester she enrolled in one of my children’s literature classes. Now she is reading more books than she has ever read before. She reports to me from time to time verbally and in her response journal that she is interested in and enjoying the reading children’s books. However, she was behind in reading the children’s literature textbook--at first because it was "harder" to read but later because she found herself so interested in the material that she had to read slowly to absorb it. My discussion with her during the interview about her reading habits and attitudes might have made her aware of what kind of a reader she was and led her to find ways of becoming a better, more willing reader. And the process of becoming more or less literate (or more or less aliterate) continues for her, just as it does for the other students involved in the study. The study I have conducted is finite; the students’ lives as readers are not. What a study can capture is only a frac- tion of reality. One of the major findings of the present study is that aliterates view themselves as individuals who do me; like to read and who avoid reading. This self—perception is 183 critical in aliteracy, a self-perception that is based on dichotomous meanings for "reader" (one either is a reader or is not). Many of the students who reported that they never read for enjoyment in reality read newspapers and/or magazines on their own. Yet, because they had viewed them selves as people who did nee read and did neg like to read, they failed to acknowledge the ways in which they did me seme degree like to read or read for enjoyment. For in- stance, Ruth said in the beginning of the interview, "sitting down and reading a book of a couple of hundred pages. That just not like me." Yet, at the time of the interview, she was engrossed in a book that was 400 pages long and feeling frustrated that she was not finding time to read it. And, like the other interviewees, she had completely ignored the magazines that she regularly read and did not consider this type of reading as pleasure reading. We can conclude that in reality those who are viewed as aliterates do like reading more than they report and do read more than they report. It would be more accu- rate, however, to say: they do not like to read as much, do not read as much, do not read as wide a range of texts, do not have as many different reasons for reading, are not as deeply motivated to read, and/or are not as adept in read- ing as those who are more avid readers. These students had not given themselves enough credit for what they did read and for the extent to which they did read for pleasure. One could say that they had a poor 184 image of themselves as readers. Those who had indicated through their questionnaire answers that they valued read- ing as an activity (as most people in this society do) felt uncomfortable about the discrepancy between their belief about reading and their perception of themselves as read- ers. Furthermore, as pointed out in Chapter 6, students in this survey who didn’t like to read tended to rate them- selves as poorer readers than those who liked to read. Those whom I interviewed tended to believe that their reading habits and attitudes had affected their schooling, had led to lower grades, and had made them less competent students. One student remarked that if he read more per- haps he would be a "better person." Feelings of guilt and insecurity also tend to be associated with their low per— ception of their reading ability. How did these low perceptions of themselves as readers come about? One explanation lies in the way they defined "reading" and in what they thought a "good reader" or a "poor reader" is. Purves (1984) and Heath (1980) have written about the differences between reading as taught in the schools and reading as practiced in real life (see a more detailed treatment of this topic in Chapter 2). Purves contends, for instance that school reading demands a series of complex activities (such as answering oral or written questions about the text) before and after reading while reading outside of school may simply consist of 185 choosing a book one likes, reading as much of it as one likes to, and maybe telling a friend, the book is worth reading. Rasinski (1989) writes that among students a "good reader“ tends to be defined in terms of reading proficiency as measured by tests and later in their school- ing in terms of ability to read textbooks in various school subjects. This definition contrasts with the defi- nition of a "good reader" in adulthood: one who reads constantly and perhaps talks about what he or she reads with others. It seems that the students who thought of themSelves as relatively poor readers judged themselves in terms of school reading standards. The frequent testing of stu- dents’s reading ability throughout their schooling may have contributed to the belief that they are deficient readers. According to some educators who have written on reading reluctance or aliteracy, children who do not read at the grade level "appropriate" for their age are treated in ways that make them feel anxious, frustrated, and discouraged about reading. For instance, they are asked to read aloud and their mistakes corrected in front of other students; they are labeled as "slow" or "remedial" and put'in reading groups, where they become self-fulfilling failures. This study’s findings appear to support the contention that school reading goals, such as the emphasis on teaching 186 discrete reading skills and frequent monitoring of stu- dents’ reading proficiency using standardized tests, are misguided and have unfortunate results. These students also seemed to compare themselves with those who are "good readers," whom they defined as people who read books all the time, people who can sit for hours at a time and devour whole books. Some of the interviewees compared themselves to the most avid readers they knew and assumed that because they didn’t read three or four books a week, they did not like to read or were not good readers. In a sense, their definition of a "good reader" is an idealized one; a "gOod reader" to them is a person who is competent in all types of reading, perhaps a person who reads a continuous text in a linear fashion, reading every word, and a person who can read textbooks with good compre- hension and retention. Rosenblatt (1978) defines two types of reading--effer- ent and aesthetic. The purpose of efferent reading is practical, to get information or knowledge from the text, while the purpose of aesthetic reading is enjoyment of the experience of reading. The belief that reading is only for the gaining of knowledge seems to be associated with alit- eracy. Furthermore, because schools stress reading for knowledge (efferent reading) more than reading for pleasure (aesthetic reading), many people think that reading is something only done for school purposes and not for its inherent value. In visit to an optometrist’s office over 187 spring break, my eight-year-old son went through the rou- tine vision tests involving the reading of letters of the alphabet projected on a wall across the room. The optome- trist quipped, "You don’t have to read. You’re not in school!" The remark amused me and also made me think: I wondered how many children and students take it seriously and actually apply it. Children learn from what they hear and from what they are taught. As many educators I cited in Chapter 3 have contended, our schools tend to stress reading to meet school requirements such as passing tests and receiving grades and deemphasize the personal uses and values of reading. Because of the prominent roles that attitude toward reading and reading ability seem to play in aliteracy, I have developed a model that shows the relationship between two components and other components of aliteracy (see Figure 10). Poor reading ability can be the cause or the effect of a negative reading attitude, just as reading attitude can be the cause or the effect of poor reading ability. In this model, reading ability and attitude toward reading have more prominent positions than the other components because they seem to start the cycle of aliteracy. Both low reading ability and negative attitude toward reading may result in few motivations for reading, low intensity of motivations for reading, low amount of 188 homuouwud ocm summons: «0 mucucomfioo . 0H ousmflm Amodha bosom uo\o:m Anumxe bosom uo\o:m ucsofid nommoav liiil xufimcuucH nozoqv \ i .V Hogwaom A: .V pace on mi/ \\ assumes no: sodom>nnoz so: I, \ / \. Kw MWMVANNHMHLUanHAN [I # sou i\. W 304 . lliiilii‘llllilillliiiii 956mm“ . 32:: Beach. . ‘I' I‘ll! I - . \u \l \i I“ :3: b. \\ a3: NUQHBHQ 3 a \ / \ Ammo»? duo: uo\o:m Anomae ouoz uo\ocm .\.\ //.II acnoEm Hocmflzv reflmQOucm umnmwmv .L‘ I. w notifier Al. .I. I... .W floor 09 ocfiomwm saw: :Oflum>fluoz £0“: 189 reading and few types of texts read. These other compo- nents, in turn, influence reading ability and attitude. The model also shows the flip side of aliteracy, which might be called, for want of a better word, literacy. However, I must point out that, technically, aliteracy represents only an aspect of literacy. It applies only to pmlnm. Literacy, as I have explained in Chapter 2 is a broader concept that goes beyond the technology of reading and writing to include a way of thinking and behaving. We can say that aliteracy is a way ef gninking end behaving teward pninp. Aliteracy is a contextual phenomenon. People’s read— ing habits and interests are affected by differentcircum- stances such as time, place, people they are with, and events they take part in. The time (whether or not it is vacation, for example) in which a survey is conducted can affect the answers given. Is aliteracy a short-term or a long-term condition? In yelees_ef_3eedere (1988), Carlsen and Sherrill distinguished between students who stopped reading temporarily as a result of activities and students who had not been reading or had not been interested in reading for a long period. Lampert and Saunders (1976) de scribe a similar group of high school students, who per- ceived themselves as "non-readers" and as people who did not like to read (although 30 percent of them did read newspapers, magazines, and occasionally novels). The 190 findings of the present study suggest that it is a leng; pemm (more or less) condition and a definite mindset about reading. The students I interviewed, who viewed themselves as individuals who did not like to read and who did not read for enjoyment, reported that they had that attitude and behavior since childhood. All of them could HQ: recall a time when they were interested in reading or when they read more than they read today. Aliteracy may reflect a negative attitude toward literacy; specifically toward literate thought and behav- ior. It may reflect a tendency to avoid information and may be a part of a general pattern of information avoid- ance. Studies in newspaper readership (Fedler and Taylor, 1978; Poindexter, 1979; Sobal and Jackson-Beeck, 1981) have identified the characteristics of non-newspaper readers. They tend to come from low socioeconomic and educational backgrounds and to consider themselves members of the lower class. They tend to be very young or very old and "to be less active and less involved with their neighbors" (Fedler and Taylor, 1978, p. 303). They have also been found to be less involved politically because they are less likely to have voted (Sobal and Jackson-Beeck, 1981, p. 13). They have been described as "information-poor" (Scherer, 1989, p. 184). Because of the characteristics that cluster around these non-newspaper readers they tend to be considered truly disadvantaged. Mass communication research has also found evidence to support the theory that 191 a relationship exists between uses of the media: "Heavy users of one medium are likely to be heavy users of anoth- er" (Scherer, 1989, p. 184). Some studies have found that many people tended to read newspaper accounts of events that they have already heard on radio or television. Other studies have shown that with growing dependence on televi- sion for information, there is less intermedia usage except for usage of newspapers and magazines (i.e., newspaper readers also tend to be magazine-readers). In Chapter 3, I cited studies that show that book readers tend to be readers of magazines and newspapers; in fact they tend to read slightly more magazines and news- papers than those who only read magazines and newspapers. Book readers also tend to be active in other recreational and community activities. All these findings suggest that aliteracy, if accompanied by these conditions, is a real cause for concern. If a person’s lack of interest in and avoidance of reading is part of a general pattern of information avoidance and uninvolvement with other people, then aliteracy is probably detrimental to that person and to society. Mass communication research lends support to the arguments eXpressed by Baroody (Thimmesch, 1984) in the following statement: Literacy has two critical functions in a pluralistic society. First, it knits a people together, giving them a common culture. Of equal importance, literacy provides people with the 192 intellectual tools used to question, challenge, understand, disagree, and arrive at consensus. In short, it allows peOple to participate in an exchange of ideas. A democratic institution is weakened when fewer and fewer citizens can par- ticipate in such an exchange. Aliteracy leads inexorably to a two tiered society: the knowl- edgeable elite and the masses. It makes a common culture illusory or impossible; it erodes the basis for effective decision making and partici- pation in the democratic process. (p. ix) Additional studies (Sobal and Jackson-Beeck, 1981, p. 10) have also revealed that an atypical group of non-news- paper readers exist, and they differ in characteristics from the typical group. They tend to be middle-aged and to belong to the middle to upper socioeconomic groups. They also tend to have more education. They report lack of time and lack of interest in reading the content of news- papers as the reasons for not reading newspapers. This group I would designate as partly aliterate, but whether their aliteracy is part of a general pattern of informa- tion-avoidance and social uninvolvement is not explored in the studies. Perhaps they are similar to the students in the present study who reported that did not like to read and did not read on their own. Perhaps they are future versions of these students. Not enough information is available on the use of other media and involvement in other activities on the-part of the participants of the present study, so I cannot draw any definite conclusion on the extent to which K.V.C.C. students’ aliteracy signifies information avoidance or social uninvolvement. However, during the interviews, I 193 did not get the impression that the interviewees spent a lot of time watching television. Perhaps, because of all the negative opinion expressed in the mass media about excessive television watching, they did not want to admit that they watched much television. Research suggests that people tend to underreport television watching (as they tend to overreport reading). Keith, the non-reader, who said that he didn’t read books, magazines or newspapers, said that he didn’t watch much television either. When asked how he found out about what was going in the world, he said that he was "lucky" in that he could catch news headlines on occasions when he did watch television brief- ly. It is doubtful that Keith is able to gain much infor- mation through such means. One might ask if his tendency to avoid print might put him in the category of "informa- tion-poor." At the 1982 aliteracy conference in Washington D.C., questions were raised that the educational experts and representatives of the publishing business present did not or could not answer in a conclusive way. Among these ques tions are: "What is the value of reading? What are we losing if we are not reading? What happens if we do not have a reading population?" (Thimmesch, 1984, p. 18). The research cited above offers partial answers to these questions by suggesting that those with aliterate charac- teristics tend to be "information-poor" and tend to avoid 194 political and social involvements. Another question raised by a member of the audience was "What does reading give us that is of some social advantage that cannot be obtained through other media?" (p. 22). There is social prestige attached to reading, as shown in people’s tendency to overreport their reading activities. One answer suggested by the research in newspaper readership is that those who do not read also tend not to use other media and that they become social isolates. Their avoidance of reading and other sources of information may imply that they tend to avoid literate thinking. Hence, in some people, aliteracy may truly be the opposite of literacy. What is the future for victims of aliteracy? Can a person’s attitude toward reading change? How hard or easy is it to change? Some people who have aliterate character- istics may not want to change their reading attitudes and/or habits. From the perspective of aliterates, alit- eracy may be not as serious as those who read and support reading imply. Some students of children’s literature who read the editorial on illiteracy and aliteracy (which I referred to in Chapter 1) remarked that they considered aliteracy not as bad a problem as illiteracy. They con- tended that aliteracy is a matter of choice, and if one satisfies one's informational and recreational needs through means other than reading, what’s wrong with that? Keith, one of the students I interviewed, argued that his needs for recreation and information were being satisfied 195 by movies, radio, and television. To him the idea of reading a book was "ridiculous" and "a big joke." He was proud of his lack of interest in reading and found no need or reason to change. The other interviewees, who consid- ered themselves deficient as readers because of their lack of inclination to read, showed varying degrees of interest in changing their attitudes and habits. Despite their wish that they read more, some were realistic enough to say that they probably would not read for enjoyment more than they did. If the need and opportunity for reading arose they would read more. But they would read for information. For instance, one student would "need" to read more about how to run a business and another student would read more about real estate. And even Keith, whose interests had widened because of his new position in his father’s company, men- tioned reading a Reader’s Digest article on management. Two students who, through their mothers’ intervention, had read novels with enjoyment, said that they wished they could find other books equally interesting. However, their motivation to find these books for themselves seemed low. A belief that they didn’t like to read and that reading is work (probably because of lack of skills, which in turn is affected by lack of reading) would take a combination of forces to overcome. A negative reading attitude plus relatively weak reading skills lead to a self-concept about reading that is 196 tenacious and hard to change. The results of the present survey have enabled me to understand better the findings of a study that I did in a class a few years ago. After a few semesters of teaching children’s literature, I sensed that students who reported negative reading attitudes in the beginning of the course seemed to become more positive in their view of reading toward the end of the course. Conse- quently, I conducted a study in which I tried to compare students’ reading attitudes at the end of the term to their attitudes in the beginning, using a Likert-type question- naire. Despite the behavioral and attitudinal changes that I had observed among many students, the results of the survey showed a disappointingly low improvement in atti- tude. One reason for the low improvement may be that, in the beginning of the term, the students might have exagger— ated their reading activity and reported reading attitudes that were more positive than they actually were. The findings of the present study offer another explanation: negative attitudes reinforced by the other components of aliteracy, especially weak reading skills, are not easily reversible. It probably takes repeated and prolonged positive experiences with reading (one semester of a course cannot do it) in addition to self-motivation, a desire to change and a host of other forces to effect a measurable change. 197 Some of the answers students gave to the question of what influenced their reading attitudes and behavior re- vealed that these students, who didn’t like to read or didn’t read for enjoyment when they were younger, had learned to like reading and to read more as they grew older. Some wrote that they discovered pleasure reading for themselves after they left school. Some blamed teach- ing practices used in the schools for their lack of incli- nation to read. The most revealing answers have come from those who have developed more positive attitudes toward reading; they appeared to have more to say and to be more willing to discuss the influences on their reading habits and attitudes than those whose attitudes toward reading were more negative. (The latter might be more reticent because, viewing reading as a socially desirable activity, they were embarassed about their habits and beliefs about reading. They might also lack metacognition or understand- ing of their reading processes and experiences.) Recemmendatlone for Educapons and Implications fog Classroom Teacnlng On the basis of the findings I have presented, I would like to recommend some approaches that may lead to a change or reversal in attitudes toward reading. Since my study involves community college students, these approaches tend to be more appropriate for older students than younger ones . 198 1. We need to encourage students to examine their beliefs and assumptions about reading. We need to help them idene tify misconceptions about reading or misunderstandings about the reading process. Because they tend to think that reading is a monolithic activity, with no differences from one reading to the next, we should stress the different types and purposes of reading. We also should help them understand the real nature of their reading problems. (One of the interviewees described her problems in reading in terms of oral reading--she could not pronounce words--and in terms of writing-~she could not spell words.) They need to consider the possibility that their problems with read- ing textbooks may be a result of the features of the text, and not of their reading ability. Their idealized view of the "good reader" as someone who is proficient in all types of reading should be challenged. They need to be aware of the false dichotomies in their thinking of reading: that readers are either "good" or "bad," that people either "love" or "hate" reading, or that reading for information is in opposition to reading for pleasure (some pleasure- readers prefer non-fiction to fiction). They need to disabuse themselves of the notion that reading for pleasure only applies to book reading; certainly reading covers a multitude of texts, including magazines and newspapers and a multitude of reasons and purposes (as Heath [1980] and Taylor [1990] have demonstrated in their research). 199 2. We need to educate people about aliteracy. Since it is not yet in the dictionary, most people are not familiar with the term. As a term and concept, however, it desig- nates a condition that most peOple seem to be aware of, either in themselves or in other people. Writing about his life as a college student, Trimmer (1990) says, "I was 18 and illiterate, or more precisely aliterate. I could read but preferred not to" (p. 157). When people find out what I am writing about in this dissertation, many of them (they tend to be women) say that they have family members who are aliterate (they tend to be men). As a label for individu- als, "aliterate" may heighten people’s anxiety about their literacy level and about their status in society. Readers are prone to lumping aliterates with illiterates and to assuming that they are socially inferior. As a label, "aliterate" needs to be used with extreme care and not applied loosely. As the present study has shown, aliteracy is a complex, multi-dimensional, and contextual phenomenon, which has different meanings and implications for different people. If used as an adjective for people, it must be used with qualifications and explanations. ‘ However, as a label, "aliterate" does not seem to have the stigma "illiterate" carries. My students, for example, appear willing to use the term to describe themselves. Pattison (1982) claims that it is llllmemeey that people in this society are nervous and embarrassed about. At the 200 very least, the existence of the term "aliteracy", if used accurately, might help educators and the public understand the exact nature of the literacy problems this society faces. By telling people about aliteracy as a concept, we give them the means to identify a condition or problem that they know exists and perhaps lead them to find solutions to it. Currently we find Mrs. Barbara Bush waging a campaign to erase illiteracy, despite evidence that true illiteracy has been almost totally eliminated in this country and that probably the "real enemy" is aliteracy. Eeeple magazine recently described the First Lady’s pet cause as "studious- ly uncontroversial" (1990, p. 88). Certainly, no one in this society would disapprove of the elimination of illit- eracy. Would the public support as enthusiastically a campaign to reduce aliteracy? If we share Pattison’s as sumption that the culture does not value full and compre- hensive literacy, we will argue that they would not. The schools (and the public) may be satisfied with basic lit— eracy because it is politically safer; to foster full literacy encouraged by wide reading may lead to the kind of "student unorthodoxy" that schools of the 18005 feared (Graff, 1987) and create citizens who think critically and who question the status quo. In other words, aliteracy has political implications, which may not be fully acceptable to society, especially to those who hold authority and power. For this reason, a campaign to eliminate alitegacy 201 may not garner the kind of support that Mrs. Bush’s illit- eracy project has received. 3. We need to help students become aware of the conse- quences of aliteracy, one of which is the deterioration of reading skills from lack of use. Because the respondents in the present study are students who are forced to read for their classes, the deterioration may not happen as long as they are reading. However, once out of school, their reading competence will tend to decline, if they continue the pattern of reading avoidance. Most of the intervie- wees reported having difficulties with reading comprehen- sion. Their comments support research findings indicating that students lack higher comprehension or literacy skills (e.g., National Assessment of Educational Progress, 1985). According to the socio-psycholinguistic or transactional theory of reading, these students lack the schemas needed to understand the content and structure of various texts (Weaver, 1988). The research findings are conclusive: the more people read, the better they read (and the more they understand.) Reading proficiency is significantly related to reading lume. Knowing that aliteracy leads to a deterioration of reading ability, people might be compelled to read more. 4. We need to stress that members of the next generation may be influenced by today’s aliterates. According to the 202 research on the role of the home environment in reading, the children of aliterates are likely to be affected by their parents’ reading habits. Parental or sibling model- ing has a strong influence on children’s reading. Smith, Smith, and Mikulecky et al. (1978; found in Mikulecky et al., 1979, p. 12) suggest that poor modeling of reading at home starts a "vicious circle." The children are influ- enced not to read and to dislike reading; consequently, their reading ability does not develop from lack of read- ing. At school they get help in improving their skills, but the poor reading habits remain. They pass their class- es, but the lesson they learn is that reading is mainly for school. Once out of school, they abandon reading, and the cycle continues. Their model is reproduced below (Miku- lecky et al., 1979, p. 12). (See Figure 11.) 5. In stressing the influence of aliteracy on future generations, we should at the same time persuade parents to use proven methods of breaking the cycle of aliteracy. One of these is reading aloud to children using children’s literature. The benefits to children of parents reading aloud to them have been documented by many studies (see Trelease, 1989 and Kimmel and Segel, 1983). However; the benefits to parents have not been stressed or researched as much. When parents read to their children, they are bound to improve their reading skills at the same time that they help their children become better readers. Because of the 203 Poor Reading Medel— (Rarely Reads-- Dislikes Reading) Abandons Reading Influences Children Once Out of School and Younger Siblings _‘_ ____ Student "Passes" But estudent with Poor Learns That Reading Reading Ability Is Mainly for School Results ‘§\\\\\\\\\~ Receives Extra Skills Training But Poor Reading Habits Remain Figure 11 "A Vicious Circle: NOnreaders produce non— readers" (Mikulecky, et al., 1979) 204 length, accessibility, and appeal (of illustrations and text) of children’s books, they make great reading for parents. A mother taking children’s literature recently told a class I was teaching that her own reading ability and interest in reading increased as a result of reading aloud to her son. We need to emphasize that reading aloud to children may not only increase reading skills but may also bring about more favorable attitudes toward reading. Many children’s books are attractive, lively, and engaging: it is easy to get hooked once one starts reading them. Because reading attitude and reading ability are inter- twined, reading aloud to children by parents may be a powerful force in reducing aliteracy. .It benefits two generations of readers. 6. As educators, we should find effective methods of increasing interest in reading enmslge_e:_eeneel. An ap- proach that I would put at the head of a list of recommend— ed methods is the use of independent reading programs, which allow students to choose what they want to read from available materials. In Chapter 6, I quoted answers writ- ten by students who learned to enjoy reading on their own after they left school. These comments reveal the impor- tance of being able to choose what one wants to read and the positive effects of choice of reading materials on attitudes toward reading. In pioneering research conducted at Ohio State University, independent reading programs were 205 found highly successful (Schatz et al., 1960). Likewise, in Heeked en Beoks (1976), Fader documents his success in using an independent reading program at a school for delin- quent boys. Teachers from primary schools through college who have allowed students to choose the books they want to read have reported marked improvement in attitudes toward reading. As a teacher of children’s literature, a course in which I allow students to choose books they want to read, I have found the following statements to be true of college students as well: When children are free to choose their own material and free to read, they set goals for themselves that surprise most adults. They read for a purpose and for different purposes. Read- ing is important to them. Their enthusiasm is genuine. They have a real desire to improve their ability to read and comprehend material of increasing difficulty. (Schatz et al., 1960, p. 69) 7. We need to discuss with students, other educators, and the public what we know about the origins of aliteracy. It is important to attend to students’ explanations of the origins of their reading attitudes and habits. As I have explained in Chapter 6, their answers coincide with educa- tors’ explanations about the possible causes of aliteracy. As educators, we should be concerned about the evidence that points to aliterates as "hidden failures of education" (Cullinan, 1989, p. 6) and to aliteracy as a by-product of our teaching practices. We defeat our own purposes if'as we teach children to read, we also teach them to dislike 206 reading. Aliteracy, in other words, is a result of educa- tion that "disables" rather than "ables" (terms used by sociologist Robert Bellah [1989] in a speech on higher education). At the same time, I believe that it is useful for educators to be aware of the cultural forces that have helped shape literacy instruction as we know it today. This knowledge will help us better understand some root causes of our literacy problems and perhaps keep us from unnecessarily castigating ourselves for our "failures." A knowledge of history will also help us identify the weak- nesses and flaws in our goals and methods and lead us to seek solutions. 8. Teaching practices that have been known to turn stu- dents away from reading should be abandoned and replaced by approaches that encourage reading. One approach that has been repeatedly criticized by students who take classes in children’s literature is the excessive and misguided use of critical analysis in high school English classes. Students talk about excruciatingly boring and "meaningless" hours of class time devoted to "dissecting" a Shakespeare play or a Dickens novel. Furthermore, the teachers who use this approach tend to assume there is one correct reading of a text. They do not give enough consideration to the stu- dents’ idiosyncratic interpretations, a sure method to silence students and to make them read passively and with little personal involvement. Many more junior high, high 207 school, and college English teachers should be exposed to the reader response or transactional approach to reading (Rosenblatt, 1978, 1983). This view of reading validates the students’ personal and spontanous responses to a text. A host of teachers who have used reader response strategies have observed and documented improvements in students’ reading attitude and interest in literature (see Corcoran and Evans, 1979). 9. School authorities and others responsible for choosing textbooks and materials for the classroom need to be aware the effects of these books and materials on students. This study has demonstrated the harmful effects of dull, poorly written textbooks on students’ attitudes toward reading. Not only do students lose interest in reading as a result of having to read dull textbooks year after year, but they also tend to bdame themselves for their lack of interest. They tend to think that the inadequacy lies in them rather than in the textbooks. They develop a self-concept that they do not like to read and that they are not "readers." This self-concept leads to low skills because they avoid reading, and the cycle goes on. Because books are our main vehicle for teaching, we must make it our primary consider- ation for choosing books whether or not they would appeal to our students. Our textbooks and materials must promote literacy, not discourage it. 208 10. As teachers of English, we need to view ourselves more as teachers of reading, with the primary responsibility of helping students become better, more interested readers, than as teachers of "literature." I agree with Brown (1987) who writes that the main aim of a literature teacher shduld be "to develop . . . [students] as readers and responders to literature" and "to encourage those who do read to read more and those who don’t read to begin read— ing" (p. 97). 11. A major criterion for evaluating the success of reading and literature programs should be how much students are reading on their own in school and out of school (see Smith, 1986, p. 263). As stated in Chapter 1, Guthrie and Seifert (1984) stress that ". . . an underrated, poten- tially fruitful criterion of education is readership." Having attended an institution in which learning to read is the central mission, do students continue reading for useful purposes afterwards? A standard for teaching is that graduates will not only acquire the capability to read, but they will choose to read for psychological, political, occupational or domestic purposes. (p. 57) Furthermore, reading avidly is the prevalent standard in determining in out—of—school settings who good readers are. Adopting this standard in our reading and literature class- es will make school reading more congruent with real life reading and give more credibility to our programs. 12. As educators, we need to base our teaching aims and methods on a comprehensive definition of literacy-"mot as 209 the mechanical skills of reading and writing but as a life-long process of learning to read and write different kinds of texts for different purposes. Neilsen (1989) defines literacy as "the process of learning to participate in necessary and personally important social, intellectual, and political contexts" and as "a life-long process of learning to read and create contextual signs in print and in society" (p. 10). Knowing that people do not read texts unless they are necessary or personally meaningful, we should stress the personal and social uses of literacy. We must remind ourselves and others that as new needs and situations arise, people may find reasons for reading. For instance, a person starting a new hobby may be com- pelled to find information in books or magazines. Or real izing that he or she is somewhat aliterate may make a person read more. At my college, one of my colleagues who found out the subject of my dissertation made a point of telling me a few weeks later that he was trying to watch television less and to read more. People who realize that they are aliterate or verging on being aliterate can change if they need to and if they want to. They are not doomed to a life of reading avoidance. ‘Understanding that litera- cy is a complex, life-long complex of learning to read and write different texts allows and encourages people to personally take control of their own literacy, to redirect 210 their energies and time into areas they want to explore, learn about, and experience through reading. 13. Educators should be literate role models for students. I recently read in a student’s journal that a male math teacher in a school where her father teaches "bragged" about not having read a book since he finished college. As a teacher, he has the capacity to influence hundreds of students--toward reading or away from reading. We must, as educators, examine our beliefs and assumptions about literacy so that we ourselves can better understand why we have the reading habits and attitudes that we have. We need to learn about how we can influence and guide our students to become more literate. 14. Ultimately, educators have to take the lead: in rede- fining literacy to include its broad uses and functions, in persuading the public to share this definition, and in initiating changes, not only in our own classrooms and schools, but throughout the educational system-~from pre- school through graduate school. Our primary aim should be promoting literacy at all levels to ensure that aliteracy does not have a chance to take root. Understanding alit- eracy from a historical perspective, reviewing the litera- ture on aliteracy and related topics, and analyzing the findings of the present study have led to the unavoidable conclusion that crucial changes in school goals and prac- tices need to be made. A decade ago, Tchudi (1980) wrote 211 about Johnny and Jane, the "fictional anti-heroes of every newspaper and magazine article on literacy," who can read and write but who would rather not. "After their last diploma, their bout with literacy will be over . . . " (p. 75), Tchudi says, expressing concern about the condition of aliteracy without using the term. Calling for sweeping changes in education, he writes in Angs_ef_Literaey (1980): "Literacy instruction cannot be improved without a major overhaul of both its aims and its methods and without a drastic alterations of teaching conditions" (p. xiii). And he added, "The time is right for dramatic changes in lit— eracy instruction" (p. xiv). The time is right for all of us in education to make these demands again--on behalf of present and future victims of aliteracy. Futune Research The combined use of quantitative and qualitative methods in the present study has proved to be particularly appropriate and productive. Quantitative tools enabled me to compare my survey findings with those of similar sur- veys. They make possible future comparisons of the results of this study with the findings of similar studies. The similarities in the findings help demonstrate the validity of the methods used in the surveys being compared. Like— wise, the interview results provided the means and direc- tions for interpreting the findings and drawing appropriate conclusions, adding to the validity of the quantitative 212 measures. The data the interviews produced also led to a more thorough understanding of aliteracy. Because of what the interviews contributed to the study, more follow-up interviews of the respondents might have been fruitful. I' believe that a methodology that combines quantitative and qualitative tools is appropriate for investigating other questions on aliteracy and literacy. ' This study has raised questions about different as— pects of aliteracy and literacy for future research. The following is a partial list of questions that need further investigation: 1. How do people with aliterate tendencies conceptualize the reading process? Their definitions of reading and their approaches to reading different texts can be further examined and understood. 2. How do avid readers differ from people with aliterate tendencies? The areas that need exploration include: their definitions of reading, their conceptualizations of the reading process, and their approaches to reading different texts. 3. What can we learn from people who disliked reading'when they were in school but learned to like it on their own or after they left school? (They seem to have a greater awareness of their conceptualizations of reading and to be more willing to give information than those who are showing 213 more aliterate characteristics.) Approaches and methods they have used that have brought about a change in their attitudes toward reading can be identified and explored. 4. Why do the males in our society lag behind the females in reading? Why do more males exhibit aliterate character- istics than females? 5. To what extent do people who exhibit various degrees of aliteracy use other media for recreation, information, and intellectual stimulation? More specifically, if people dislike reading and generally avoid reading as a source of information, to what extent can they be categorized as "information-poor"? 6. Is there a counterpart for aliteracy in the area of writing? What is the relationship between aliteracy and its writing counterpart? 7. When parents read aloud to children, what are the effects on parents’ reading skills and attitudes? 8. What are the effects of various reading assessment methods on aliteracy? For example, what is the relation- ship between standardized testing and aliteracy? What are the relationships between different criteria for assessment and aliteracy? 214 9. What definitions of reading are implied in our goals and approaches to English and reading instruction? 10. What is the relationship between aliteracy and the use of such approaches to literature as critical analysis and reader response? Finding answers to these questions will enhance our understanding of literacy and aliteracy. In this study I have attempted to demonstrate the extent, nature, and implications of the problem of aliteracy among community college students in this country. In addition, we need to know about aliteracies among other groups and compare them with what we have discovered about aliteracy among communi- ty college students. Answers to these questions need to be shared with the public, for whom literacy, if not aliteracy, seems to be a major concern. Through understanding the problem, par- ents, educators, and the public can work together to make aliteracy a less disabling force in individual lives and in society. APPENDICES APPENDIX A: THE SURVEY QUESTIONNAIRE Dear Students: I am asking you to answer questions about the reading that you do at school and at home. Your participation in this study will add to our understanding of the reading habits and attitudes of community college students. As you complete this questionnaire, please remember that there are me right and wrong answers. As long as you are responding to the questions honestly, your answers are correct. Your answers and your participation in this study will not affect your grade in this class. If you do not wish to participate, please tell your teacher now. If you wish to participate, please sign your name on the bottom part of this sheet. After you have completed this questionnaire, you may be asked, at a future date, to respond to addition- al questions. Your answers will be kept confidential. Thank you for your help! Raelyn Joyce KVCC Instructor I have chosen to complete the questionnaire. Name Course Title __, Instructor _, 215 216 1. First, I'd like to get a general idea about the specific kinds of things you do for recreation or to relax. I have a list of activities people sometimes do. Please think over the past month since As you read each activity, please circle "yes" if you have done it this past month and "no" if you haven't. Did you a. Go to a movie? Yes No b. Dine at a restaurant for pleasure? Yes No c. Engage in a sport like bowling or fishing? Yes No d. Read for pleasure? Yes No e. Go for a walk or hike? Yes No f. Watch television? Yes No Go to a theater or concert? Yes No h. Go window shopping? Yes No 2. Now I have some questions about reading for pleasure. Many people have reported that their schedules are too busy for them to find time to read for pleasure. Please think over the past month since a. Did you happen to read any magazines? Yes No b. Did you happen to read any newspapers? Yes No c. Did you happen to read any book, either hardcover or paperpack (not including required reading for courses)? If you have started but not finished a book, that counts too. Yes No If you have read or started to read a book, what was it about? What was the title, if you can remember? If you have read other books, please list their titles or what they were about: 217 3. How often do you read for enjoyment in your spare time? (Put a check before the right answer.) Never Less than 1 time a week 1-2 times a week More than 2 times a week ||l 4. When, as nearly as you can recall, did you last read any kind of book (n: a paperback (not required reading for courses)--all theggay through? Write the month and year: What was the book about? What was the title, if you can remember? 5. Do you read magazines regularly-- - on a daily or weekly basis? Yes No (If no, please skip question #6. Go on to question #7.) 6. If you read magazines regularly, which of the following types of magazines do you read regularly--on a daily or weekly basis? Newsmagazines, such as Newsweek or Time Yes No Household or family magazines, such as People, Good Housekeeping, Reader's Digest Yes No Sports publications, such as Sports Illustrated Yes No Personal health, self-improvement, or fashion magazines Yes No Hobby magazines Yes No Religious or church magazines Yes No Business or trade magazines Yes No Others: 7. Do you read at least one newspaper regularly (on a daily or weekly basis)? Yes No Please write the titles of newspapers that you read regularly: 8. 218 Please indicate how you feel about each of the following statements bY indicating whether you--strongly agree diSIQree. strongly disagree, or have no opinion. agree , Reading is a useful activity. i I like to read. 1 My friends would be surprised to see me 1 buying or borrowing a book that is not 5 required for class. While waiting in a doctor's office, or in an airport, or in a supermarket, I find myself leafing through magazines or paperback books. I think reading books takes too much time and concentration. Even though I'm a very busy person, I always somehow find time for reading. One of my first impulses is "to look it up" whenever there is something that I don't know or whenever I'm about to start a new task, activity, or project. I usually don't read unless I have to. I find myself giving special books to friends and relatives as gifts. m-' ———~ Jame—g . -l I choose to read non-required books and articles fairly regularly (a few times a week). Sometimes I find myself so excited about a book or an article or a story, I try to get friends to read it. ._ ~—--———— —. _ _--_.. ____. .- Reading is not that important outside of school) When I was young, I read a great deal about ; certain topics. i »_._.~— __ When I find an author I really like, I sometimes try to read the other books written by that author. I usually do all the required reading for courses. 219 9. As a reader, I consider myself: (Please put a check above the right answer.) Poor Below Average Average Above Average 10. Now. please rate from "Very Unlike Me" to "Very a scale from 1 to 5, your reasons for reading. EkEETTent Like Me", on a. I read-~to find out how to get something done. Very Like Me Very Unlike Me «a l 2 3 4 S b. I read--to keep up with what's going on. Very Like Me . Very Unlike Me 1 2 3 4 5 c. I read--to discuss what I have read with friends. Very Like Me Very Unlike Me 1 2 3 4 S d. I read—~for relaxation and personal enjoyment. Very Like Me Very Unlike Me 1 2 3 4 5 e. I read--to study for personal and occupational advancement. Very Like Me . Very Unlike Me 1 2 3 4 5 11.. How much of the following types of reading do you do these 'days? (Put a check above the right answer.) a. Job-related reading b. Textbook-reading A lot A moderate amount A little None A lot A moderate amount A little None c. Light book reading A lot A moderate amount A little None d. Magazines A lot A moderate amount A little None e. Newspapers A lot A moderate amount A little None f. Others (Please specify.): A lot A moderate amount A little None 220 12. Compared to other people my age, my understanding or comprehension of things I read is-- Poor Below Average Average Above Average Excellent 13. Compared to other people my age, my rate or speed of reading is-- Poor Below Average Average Above Average Excellent 14. Why do you think you are the kind of reader that you are today? What or who might have influenced your reading attitudes and habits? For example, can you recall past experiences, wheth- er they occurred at home or at school, that might explain why you feel the way you do about reading? (Please don't worry about spelling, grammar, or nwchanics when you write. Just write as though you were talking to another person.) 22] 15. So that I can see how your opinions compare with those of other people, I would like a few facts about you. a. What is your sex? Male Female b. In what year were you born? c. What is-the highest level of education you obtained? Elementary school or less Some high school High school graduate Some college College graduate Post graduate degree d. Are you presently a full-time student a part-time student e. Are you ____ White/Caucasian Black/African-American Oriental Hispanic Native American Other: THANK YOU FOR YOUR COOPERATION! APPENDIX B: APPROVAL FOR HUMAN SUBJECTS RESEARCH MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY UNIVHSITY COMMITTEE ON RESEARCH INVOLVING EAST LANSING ° MICHIGAN 0 «824-1!!! HUMAN SUBJECTS (UCIIHS) 206 BERKEY HALL (5”) 333-973. April l7, l990 IRB# 90-151 Raelyn A. Joyce l920 Hillsdale Kalamazoo, MI 49007 Dear Ms. Joyce: RE: "A STUDY OF ALITERACY AMONG COMMUNITY COLLEGE STUDENTS IRB# 90-151“ The above project is exempt from full UCRIHS review. The proposed research protocol has been reviewed by another committee member. The rights and welfare of human subjects appear to be protected and you have approval to conduct the research. You are reminded that UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year. If you plan to continue this project beyond one year, please make provisions for obtaining appropriate UCRIHS approval one month prior to April l7, l99l. Any changes in procedures involving human subjects must be reviewed by UCRIHS prior to initiation of the change. UCRIHS must also be notified promptly of any problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects during the course of the work. Thank you for bringing this project to my attention. If I can be of any future help, please do not hesitate to let me know. Sincerely, J n K. Hudzik, PhD. Chair, UCRIHS JKH/sar cc: 8. Tchudi ZZZ MSU a an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institutio- LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Alexander, J. E. and Filler, R. C. (1976). Attitudes and reading. Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association. Anders, P. and Cardell, D. (1977). Self-perceptions and reading habits of adolescents, college students, and adolescents. (ERIC Report No. ED 149 324) Anderson, R., Fielding, L., and Wilson, P. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Reaearcn Quarterly, 23, 285-303. Anzalone, S. J. and McLaughlin, S. D. (1983). Making literacy york: the literagy approach. Amherst, MA: Center for International Education, University of Massachusets. ‘ Applebee, A. (1962). Tragirign and reform in rne taagning or English. Urbana, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English. ~ Barker, R. and Escarpit, R. (Eds.). (1973). T e book hunger. Paris: UNESCO. Barmore, J. M. and Morse, P. S. (1979). Developing lifelong readers in the middle schools. In J. L. Thomas and R. M. Loring (Eds.) Mgtivating children and yonng adnlra tg read (pp. 75-83). Phoenix: Oryx Press. Bleich, D. (1988). Tne double perspectiva; langnage, literagy, and §ocia1 relatigna. New York: Oxford University Press. Book Industry Study Group, Inc., The (1978). The l97§ 5 ch d n ' and Bo Purchaaing: a stngy inquiring inrg rna narnra gr raading ang bggk haying habits of fine Anerican nablig. Darien, Connecticut: Prepared by Yankelovich, Skelly and White for the The Book Industry Study Group. 223 224 Book industry study finds U.S. still is a nation of readers. (1978). Library Jgnrnai 10;, 2466. Boorstin, D. J. (1984). Books in our future. Washington D.C.: Library of Congress. Brown, L. (1987). Rendering literature accessible. In B. Corcoran and E. Evans (Eds.) Reagers, Texre, ang Teachers (pp. 93-118). Upper Montclair, New Jersey: Boynton Cook. Carlsen, R. G. and Sherrill, A. (1988). Voices gf readere: how we cgne to iove books. Urbana, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English. Casteel, C. A. (1989). Motivating reluctant readers to become mature readers. Reading Improyement 26, 98-102. Chambers, A. (1969). The reluctant reader. Oxford: 'Pergamon Press. Chambers, A. (1983). Introducing bogke no children. Boston: The Horn Book Incorporated. Ciani, A. J. (1981). Motivaring reluctanr'readers. Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association. Cochran-Smith, M. (1984). The making of a reader. Norwood, New Jersey: Ablex Publishing Company. Cole, John Y. and Gold, Carol 5. (1978). Reading in America. Washington D.C.: Library of Congress. Corcoran, B. and Evans E. (Eds.). (1987). Readers, tents, and teachers. Upper Montclair, New Jersey: Boynton Cook. Cullinan, B. E. (1989). Literature and the cnil . 2nd ed. New York: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich. Decker, B. C. (1985). Aliteracy: What teachers can do to keep Johnny reading." Journal gf Teagher Education, a1, 55-58. ' Dixon, J. (1967). Growth through English. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Faas, P. S. .(1989). Ontside in; Minorities ang rhe tranefgrmation of American egngation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 225 Faar, B. (1981). Building language experiences for reluctant readers. In A. J. Ciani (Ed.), Mariyaring IleQL§n£_readere. (1981) Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association. Fader, D. (1976). e ne 00 d b . New York: Berkeley Publishing Company. Fedler, F. and Taylor, P. (1978). Broadcasting’s impact on selection of news stories by readers. Jo i Qnarterly, 55, 301-305, 333. Galtung, J. (1981). Literacy, education and schooling--for what? In H. J. Graff (Ed.), Literagy and social development in the Weep: a reddgg (PP. 271-276). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. . Goetz, J. P. and LeCompte, M. G. (1984). Ethnography and gualitative design in edugational research. New York: Academic Press. Goodlad, J. (1984). A lace c 1 ed 5 0 ° S cts o the rnture. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Graff, G. (1979). The literacy myth; Lireracy and egcial tructure ' the n' et en -c ur c't . New York: Academics Press. Graff, G. (1987). Professing literatnre: an institntignal history. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Graff, H. J. (1987). The legacies gf liheragy; Conrinuiry and gontradigrione in geetern gnltnre and sggiety. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University. Greaney, V. (1980). Factors related to amount and type of leisure reading, Beading_3esearch_guarterlxi_I§. 337- 357. Guthrie, J. T. (1980). Reading in New Zealand: achievement of volume. Reading Research Quarrerly. l7, 6-27. Guthrie, J. T. and Seifert, M. (1983). Profiles of reading activity in a community. Jgprnal of Reading. 2d, 498-508 Guthrie, J. T. and Seifert, M. (1984). Meaauring readership: rationale and rechnigne. Paris: UNESCO. Harman, D. (1987). Illiteragyz a national dilenn . New York: Cambridge Book Company. 226 Harste, J. C. and Mikulecky, L. J. (1984). The context of literacy in our society. In A. C. Purves and O.Niles (Eds.). Becoming readers in a gompler society. Eighty-third yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press . . Heath, S. Brice. (1985). Being literate in America: a sociohistorical perspective. In J. A. Niles and R. V. Lalik (Eds.). Issues in literacy: a reeearch perspective (pp. 1—18). Rochester, New York. Heath, S. B. (1980). The functions and uses of literacy. Journal gf Communicationl 30, 123-133. Heather, P. (1982.). Young people's reading: the leisure reading of 13-15 year olds. No. ED 225 130) _ Hillerich, R. L. (1976). Toward an assessable definition of literacy. The English Jgurnall 65, 50-55. - (1982). ERIC/RC8 report: Motivating Holbrook, H. T. reluctant readers: A gentle push. Language ArtsI 59, 385-88. a study of (ERIC Report Howell, J. F. and Sylvester, T. A. (1983). Improving reading motivation. (ERIC Report No. ED 227 454). International Reading Association and National Council of Teachers of— English. (1989). Cases in literacy: an agenda for discussion. Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association. In the eye of the storm (1990, October) People. pp. 86-88. Jacobs, H. C. (1985). Readers--an endangered species? Vital Speeches of the Day, 51, 446-448. (1978). Motivating reluctant readers. Journal I Johns, J. L. of Research and Development in Educationl l1, 69-74. Joseph, S. and Wittig, D. L. M. eds. (1980). Reading remedies for involving the reluctant reader. (ERIC Report No. ED 192 276) Kaestle, C. F. (1988). The history of literacy and the history of readers. In E. R. Kintgen, B. M. Kroll and M. Rose (Eds.) Perspectives on Literacy. Carbondale, Illinois: Southern Illinois University Press, 95-126. 227 Kantor, K. G., Kirby, D. R., & Goetz, J. P. (1981). Research in context: Ethnographic research in English education. Rese C 0 S 293- 309. I Kazemek, F. E. (1988). Necessary changes: professional involvement in adult literacy programs. Harvard Educational Reyiew, 53, 464-83. Kimmel, M. M. and Segel, E. (1988). F0 New York: r reading out loud! Delacorte. Kirsch, I. S. and Yungeblut, A (1986). iteragy: profile of America’s ygung adults. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service. Kozol, J. (1980). Pr 'sone s o s' e t e nds of adult illiteragy in rhe unired Sheree. New York: Continuum Publishing Corporation. Kozol, J. (1985). illiterate America. Garden City, New Jersey: Anchor Press. Lehr, F. (1985). A portrait of the American as reader. dournal of Reading, , 170-173. Levine, K. (1982). Functional literacy: fond illusions and false economics. a vard c t'on v' w. 52, 249- 66. Lampert, K. and Saunders, E. (1976) . Readers and non- readers: What's the difference? Englieh Jonrnal, 65, 34-38. Langer, J. A. (1984).L1teracy instruction in American schools: problems and perspectives. American Jgnrnal of Educarign. 9;, 107- 32. . Langer, J. A. (1987). A sociocognitive perspective on literacy. In J. A. Langer (Ed.). Langnage, lireraey, t re: 'ssues o ' 20). and sc 0 lin (pp. 1- Norwood, New Jersey: Ablex Publishing Corporation. Maxwell, J. (1977). R r e f 1 ° a u ve o a tainment nd e ch' ractices i Scotlan . UK: Research. National Foundation for Educational MCEvoy, G. F. and Vincent, C. S. (1980). Who reads and why? JOurnal pf Communication, 39 134-140. 228 Mikulecky, L. (1978). Aliteracy and a changing view of reading goals. (ERIC Report No. Ed 157 052) Mikulecky, L. J., Shanklin, N. L. and Caverley, D. C. (1979). dult re din 'ts att'tude mgtivations: a crges-segtional stndy. Monograph in Language and Reading Studies. Bloomington, Indiana: School of Education, Indiana University. National Academy on Education Commission on Reading. (1985). Beco in a n t' o r rs: e re ort of the Commissign on Reading. Washington D. C.: The National Institute of Education. National Assessment of Educational Progress. (1981). ea 'n T i k’n and Writ n R su ts fro the 1979-80 N tional Ass ssm nt of. d n n ite re (Report No. 11-L-01). Denver, Colorado: Education Commission of the States. National Assessment of Educational Progress. (1985). The readi re ort d: P es ow d e ce nce ' o echgole. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service. Neilson, L. (1989). Lit rac liv' t lit at lives of three adulr . Portsmouth, New Hampshire: Heinemann Educational Books. Nell, V. (1988). st ' a ook° th s h l f r 'n for pleaeure. New Haven: Yale University Press. Noland, R. G. and Craft, L. H. (1976). Methods to motivate the reluctant reader. Journal of Reading, l9, 387—392. Ohmann, R. (1985). Literacy, technology, and monopoly. Wiser—9.. 675-89- Oxenham, J. (1980). Liter ‘ ' a 'n n soci l grganiaarign. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Paris, S. and Wixson, K. (1987). The development of literacy: Access, Acquisition, and Instruction. In D. Bloome (Ed.). Literagy and eghggling. (pp. 35-54). Norwood, NJ. . Pattison, R. (1982). On literacy; the pglitigs pf rhe word from hemer to rhe age of rank. New York: Oxford University Press. Poindexter, P. M. (1979). Daily newspaper non-readers: why they don’t read. dgnrnalism Qnarherly, 56, 764-770. 229 Purves, A. C. (1984). The potential and real achievement of U.S. students in school reading. hmerigan_dgnrnal_gr EQEQéEiQni_2;= 82-106- Ravitch, D. and Finn, C E., Jr. (1987).17- l ow? na ‘0 l assesement of hietgry ando lireratu ure. New York: Harper and Row. Resnick, D. P. and Resnick, L. B. (1977). The nature of literacy; a historical exploration. Harvard Edncational Review, 47, 370-85. Rist, R. C. (1977). On the relations among educational research paradigms: From disdain to detente. Anrhrgpology and Edugation Qnarterly. 8, 42-49. _ .Robinson, Jay. (1985). Literacy in the department of English. College EnglishII 47, 482-98. Robinson, Jay. (1990). sa io on e ' t n : W Portsmouth Hew Hampshire: Boynton-Cook. Robinson, John. (1980). The changing reading habits of the American public. doprnal pf gommunigatign, 39, 141- 152. Rosenblatt, L. (1978). The reader, the LQES. the pgem. Carbondale, Illinois: Southern Illinois University Press. Rosenblatt, L. (1983). Lite u e s e at' (4th ed.). New York: Modern Language Association of America. Schatz, E. E., Utterback, R., Wilsberg, M. E., and Frazier, A. (1960). I de e R t Primary gradee. r(Study of Independent Reading Bulletin No. 2). Columbus: The Ohio State University. Scherer, C. L. (1989). The videocassette recorder and information inequity. J ic t‘ 39, 94-103. Scribner, S. and Cole, M. (1981). The psycholggy gfi literacy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Scribner, S. (1988). Literacy in three metaphors. In E. R. Kintgen, B. M. Kroll, and M. Rose. (Eds.). Pe e ' ' . (pp. 71-91). Carbondale, Illinois: Southern Illinois University Press. '— 230 Sharon, A. (1973-74). What do adults read? Reading_Re§eareh QQ§r§§£l¥i_2. 148-169- Sloan, G. Davis. (1984). The childa ae grit ig, Te eaching W New York: Teachers College Press. Smith, F. (1983). Essays intg litera y. Portsmouth, New Hampshire: Heinemann Educational Books. Smith, F. (1986). sul t ' te ' ence° t u ea inv aeign of gur schools. Portsmouth, New Hampshire: Heinemann Educational Books. Sobol, J. and Jackson-Beeck, M., Newspaper nonreaders: a national profile. Journalism Qparrerly, 58, 9-13, 28. Sudman, S. and Bradburn, N. M. (1983). ASKiEg_QQ§§§iQE§L MW San Francisco: Josey-Bass Publishers. - Sullivan, J. L. (1985). An antidote for aliteracy--people who can read but won’t. (ERIC Report No. ED 255 934) Sutherland, Z. Monson, D. L. and Arbuthnot, M. L. (1991). thldren and Books. (8th ed.). New York: Harper Collins. Szwed, J.F. (1981). The ethnography of literacy. In E. R. Kintgen, B. M. Kroll, and M. Rose. (Eds.). Pe s 'ves o 'tera . (pp. 71-99). Carbondale, Illinois: Southern Illinois University Press. Taylor, D. and Dorsey-Gaines, C. .(1988). Growing Portsmouth, New Hampshire: Heinemann Educational Books. Taylor, D. (1990). Teaching without testing: Assessing the complexity of children’ s literacy training. Englieh EQEQ§_iQni_22, 4 74. Taylor, M. (1978). Are college students reading? Wisconein §LflLQ_A§§Q§i§£iQB_QQQLQ§l1__§p 11-15. Tchudi, S. (1980). The the of literaey; a guide fer parents_and_edngargr§. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ‘ Thimmesch, N. (Ed.). (1984). ' c ' w ’t. Washington: American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research. 231 Thomas, J. L. and Loring, R. M. (Eds. L (1979). Mehiyehing c e n on adu s to ea Phoenix: Oryx Press. Thompson, M. E. (1987). What happens when readers do not read? The problem of reluctant readers. (ERIC Report No. ED 286 165) Tanner, R. (1987). Teaching Freshmen Non-readers, the A-literate Majority. (ERIC Report No. ED 280 076) Trelease, J. (1989). The new read-aleng handbook. New York: Penguin Books. Trimmer, J. (1990). Telling stories about stories. Teeehing n ish ' wo-Year Col e e 7, 157-164. ,Tuman, M. C. )1987). Pr a t 'te ° ' ' ' to pedagogy. preetiee, and nrggress. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press. Van Allan, R. (1965).Atrituge§ end the Art ef Teaehing Reed ing. Washington D.C.: National Education Association. Venezky, R. L. Kaestle, C.F. and Sum, A.M. The_§nhrie da : ef ecti ns 0 the lite c ' ' i o Anerieen young adults. Princeton, NJ: Center for the Assessment of Educational Progress. Walberg, H. and Shiowling T. (1984). Reading achievement and diminishing returns to time. Jou na of W 442- 451. Weaver, C. (1988). Reading process and preerieez from ' - ' ' ' t w e . Portsmouth, New Hampshire: Heinemann Educational Books, Inc. Weaver, C. (1989). Basals and Reading Instruction: Do they promote or thwart literacy? highigen__3ngiieh WI 3'9 0 Williams, R. (1977). Marxism end Literetnre. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Willinsky, J. (1991). e Ne 't ° d eng writing in the scheols. London: Routledge. Wilson, M. (1988). Testing and literacy: a contradiction in terms?" In J. D. Beard and S. E. McNabb (Eds. ), ~s ' . f: t:- no_i 1 . .g..- ._ s' 1;- ehneee. (pp. 12- -16). Rochester, Michigan: Michigan Council of Teachers of English. 232 Winkle, A. W. (1988). Research on aliteracy: why Johnny doesn’t read. Ohie Begging Teaeher. 22, 40-47. . LIBRQRIES l. willlllllm 11"»)..7 7T1..)..n-:-I~n-x.4 Al'.lll~l',41|...‘III‘:IIIIIIII,I