vyu 7" .a u" a?" ‘« v: @‘a x: g -1 3N9: '2': ?'%HW?#\: $5. Vain}:- \ $22}. 1.5. . C 3' ‘ \ \ a >41 .,_, ‘ ““66,“ "'v 0 ’ ‘l x \ ,. ,‘;);';k i: . :5 3A ¢ ,3 V455. .> if“? ,3; g , . in. ‘2' v ,-"a‘=‘,{£:1 t1 , ‘ "1-"3’ r. 2,», q -» . “‘19 s.‘ . .3, . . afs§c’suffifl(% ’ "' ”is ,l .A .of g. Vat-{3.2L h _ ’11.: “AW. .n_ . 2 " '3 ,1. , . .fifi‘ru Nfiv'quy 3:0 for: ”M7" .7: w‘ gar“? [5.1; ' Chifié, '5 ' rv-_ -; A. k v‘. I 1" 'G‘*-«¢«-:»: as“, 5. ;, r1 5-45.33. ‘ If ”\t .u ..., P 5" "‘ v.12... ,7." V a, w ’5‘. -v '37 km. ,. ' ‘I "" -v 7 I a ,5- ' .30» ..-..v. ‘ . ../, ' ‘ ‘ “if! AI... 4 1 . -. ‘ P 53%;": 'x .1 r: . ' I "" ‘ t. , g“ u. . Y H “ ”was“, .. k ‘ .xjw.’ ”A." .3.‘ . var-I- ”£5?” IlllllHlllHill!llltlHHlllllllllHlHlilil‘lllllfljLHlllll 254%80014: 3 1293 00786 LIBRARY Michigan State University K I This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Locus of Control, Gender, and Social Support As Stress Moderators in Industrial Injury and Adaptation to Disability presented by Elaine M. Tripi has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D . degree m Counseling . Educational Psychology and Special Education [/ M/f/‘v Major professor Date%/)9}/ 37’ MS U is an Affirmative Action/EqualvOpponuniry Institution 0- 12771 I J | PLACE II RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or bdore due due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE __ II II LJ L_ I?! QM; l-l II : :LJE: MSU Is An Affirmuive Action/Equal Opportunity Imam cm ”3'93 \{FJ LOCUS OF CONTROL. GENDER. AND SOCIAL SUPPORT AS STRESS MODERATORS IN INDUSTRIAL INJURY AND ADAPTATION TO DISABILITY By Elaine M. Tripi A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Special Education 1989 imam loo—l ABSTRACT LOCUS OF CONTROL. GENDER, AND SOCIAL SUPPORT AS STRESS MODERATORS IN INDUSTRIAL INJURY AND ADAPTATION TO DISABILITY By Elaine M. Tripi The purpose of this study was to investigate from a multi-dimensional perspective, the relationship between stress and adjustment. Specifically, locus of control orientation (personality variable), gender (biological variable), and social support (social situation variable) were measured to determine direct and interaction effects in a group of industrially injured workers. The theoretical base for this study was drawn from rehabilitation, community, and personality psychology. Subjects for this study were 56 men and women who were currently receiving worker’s disability compensation benefits. All subjects were volunteers 18 years of age or older, who had sustained a work-related injury. and had been disabled for at least six months. but no. longer than twenty-four months. Data were collected through the use of questionnaires during a rehabilitation evaluation. A correlational design examined all variables of interest in the study. The findings are discussed in terms of their implications regarding injured workers and adjustment to disability. Results indicated that workers in this study were (a) moderately coping with their disabilities. (b) external in their locus of control orientation, and (c) minimally depressed. There were no gender differences established on any of the variables. However, gender was the only variable that had an interactive or modifier effect on the outcome variable of depression. Females were less depressed than males under high stress, while males experienced less depression under low stress. There was no main effect of gender with either of the outcome variables in this study. In addition, there was a statistically significant interaction effect of the variables with each other in regard to depression. Specifically there was a STRESS X GENDER X LOCUS OF CONTROL interaction, as well as a STRESS X GENDER X LOCUS OF CONTROL X SOCIAL SUPPORT interaction. Locus of control had a statistically significant main effect for both coping and depression. indicating that internal subjects experienced higher coping and less depression regardless of the level of stress. A statistically significant main effect was demonstrated for social support on coping. Specifically. subjects with high social support were able to cope under stress more effectively than subjects who had low social support. Future research must address this complex stress process more intensely in order to delineate and clarify issues relating to methodology and research design. DEDICATED TO GINA AND W.E.B. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This research is an outgrowth of a class taught by Dr. Robert Caldwell entitled ”Coping with Stress.‘ I initially believed this class would help me cope with the stress of completing my doctorate. Instead. it helped me conceptualize my research involving disabled workers. Special thanks to my committee members; Dr. James Engelkes for his academic assistance throughout the years, Dr. Donald Galvin for his support and enthusiasm in this project, Dr. William Hinds for getting me through the administrative hurdles, and my academic mentor, Dr. Robert Caldwell. His advice and support have been immeasurable in the entire research process. I am indebted to Yeager and Company for allowing me to utilize their files for my data. Their cooperation was so much appreciated. I also wish to thank the Genesis Group for allowing me to conduct my research at their facility. Special thanks to Karen Cruise and Brian Kalt for their technical assistance. My family has been a very critical part of my life and I want to acknowledge the support that I have received from my mother, sisters and brother. Knowing that we share a bond of caring and love has made me a stronger and better human being. My love and thanks to my daughter, Gina who has grown into a beautiful young lady during my doctoral endeavor. Finally, I want to thank W.E.B. for always being there and loving me. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Disability as a Stressful Life Event Adaptation to Stress: Locus of Control as a Moderator Variable Social Support as a Moderating Variable Gender as a Moderating Variable Interaction of Moderator Variables Purpose of the Study Research Hypotheses Research Model Overview ll METHODOLOGY Introduction Subjects Sampling Procedures Site/Setting Apparatus/Interview Instrumentation Research Team Procedures III RESULTS Introduction Sample Characteristics on the Variables in the Study Tests of the Major Hypotheses of the Study IV SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Introduction Discussion Limitations Conclusions Implications for Future Research vi viii ix 42 42 42 44 61 61 61 75 77 79 APPENDICES APPENDIX A APPENDIX B APPENDIX C APPENDIX D APPENDIX E APPENDIX F APPENDIX G APPENDIX H APPENDIX I LIST OF REFERENCES Consent to Participate Form Stressful Life Events Questionnaire lntemal/Extemal Locus of Control Scale Beck Depression Scale Social Support Dimension Scale Coping With Disability Inventory Logbook Introductory Participation Statement UCRIHS Approval Letter vii 80 83 96 103 109 1 17 130 132 134 136 Table 1 Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Table 12 Table 13 LIST OF TABLES Educational levels of industially injured workers in the study. 28 Occupational distribution of industrially injured workers in 29 the study. Income levels of industrially injured workers in the study. 31 She‘mparison of stress levels over time for subjects in the 45 y. Summary table of sample characteristics on the variables of 47 interest in the study. Multiple resression for locus of control with depression and 49 coping. Multiple regression for social support with depression and 52 coping. Multiple regression for gender with depression and coping. 55 Multiple regression for locus of control, social support, and 56 gender with depression and coping. Summary table of results of statistical analyses of hypotheses 59 of the study. Mean BDI scores for males and females under high and low 70 stress. Mean BDI scores for stress, gender, and locus of control. 72 Breakdown: Mean BDI scores for stress, gender, locus of 74 control and social support. viii Figure 1 Figure 2 Figure 3 Figure 4 LIST OF FIGURES Conceptual model of vulnerability hypothesis. Conceptual model of modified vulnerability hypothesis. Marital status of industrially injured workers participating in the study. Race distribution of industrially injured workers in the study. ix Chapterl Mon Stress research and studies examining adaptation to stress (i.e., general adaptation syndrome studies) have proven to be quite popular during the past several decades. Pioneering work by Selye (1956) initially focused on stress response. He has described three stages which the body passes through in fighting to maintain/retain its natural inclination to homeostasis or harmony. These stages constitute what is known as the l ti Syndrome (GAS) and are listed below: $13114: Alarm Reaction (ARS). The ARS occurs when the body/Individual initially perceives a threat/stress is imminent. At this stage, widespread circulatory and digestive changes occur, as well as increased adrenal and pituitary gland secretions. W: Stage of Resistance (RESS). At the RESS, the body increasingly makes adjustments to the continuing threat/stressor. as production of secretions of the adrenal and pituitary gland rises above its normal level. Further, the body may exact a price for this adaptation by the onset of stress-related diseases such as high blood pressure, depression, ulcers, headaches, etc. (i.e.. diseases of adaptation). StageJfl: State of Exhaustion (EXHS). At this final stage, the body has finally grown incapable of responding to the demands of the stressorfthreat in a healthy manner. Consequently. at this stage the pituitary and adrenal glands have been strained beyond their limits. Rates of secretion that had increased at the RESS stage can no longer be maintained. So serious is this stage that the body’s inability to fight has led to fatigue and exhaustion. The end result can be death itself. Subsequent research has extended the base of knowledge relative to 2 these phenomena. Emphasis has been given to the manner in which life events produce dysfunctional behavior and/or emotional disorder in individuals. While most researchers agree there is a casual link between recent life events and illness symptomatology, outcome studies tend to show weak results. Correlation studies have suffered much of the same fate. It is thought that the reason for this lack of robust findings is due to the presence of other factors which may interact in this process, as well as the cumulative effect of individualistic responses. While Selye’s theory (1973) has focused on biological adaptation to stress, Lazarus, Cohen, Folkman. Kanner, and Schaefer’s (1980) theory of stress has tended to focus on the psychological. Lazarus et al., ( 1980) contend that psychological stress is dependent on the degree to which individuals can successfully interaCt with their environment. Lazarus proposes that the more successfully the person can transact with the environment, the more the stress can be moderated and less stress will be felt. Lazarus and colleagues (1980) have also offered that the link between stresses and disease should be examined by way of a tri-Ievel analysis: the physiological level (as proposed by Selye), the psychological , and the social. It is assumed that a response pattern is initiated on the part of individuals when a stressful life event occurs. Responses to that event may then be moderated by preexisting personal dispositions and social experiences as well as certain socioeconomic status variables. These moderators or variables that affect the way individuals perceive and respond to stressful life events can significantly enhance and explain the stress illness process. Caldwell, Pearson, and Chin (1984) suggested that three classes of variables have been shown to moderate the effect of stressful life events or adaptation to that event. They are as follows: 1. Personality variables such as autonomy/Intemality and/or passivity/extemality. 2. Social factors, such as social support. 3. Biologic/genetic variables. such as age or sex. According to Mitzel (1982), disability is a chronic or long-term condition that results from an injury or disease. A broader definition suggested by Parmeggiani (1983) is that disability is any restriction or lack (resulting from impairment) of ability to perform an activity in the manner considered normal for a human being. Expression may be in the form of depression, sleep disturbances, substance abuse disorders, as well as physical manifestations. From whatever causative agent, and regardless as to expression. disability can be viewed as a stressful life event (Mitzel, 1982). Peterson and Seligman (1983) suggested that beyond the physical expression of suffering and loss of wages, the disabled adult may also show profound impairments extending as far as the psychological realm. While they contend that these impairments are not necessarily pathological, Peterson and Seligman (1983) asserted that such psychological impairments may leave the disabled adult feeling that the situation (injury) is well beyond their ability to cope. One can question the degree to which such feelings of passivity and lack of control are a situational reaction to the event, or a generalized personality response that may hinder positive adaptation (i.e.. successful utilization of individual skills and capacities to transcend the stressful life event) (Mechanic, 1976). Seligman (1975) in describing his theory of "learned helplessness” postulated that people who cannot control the aversive events in their lives become depressed. In fact. Seligman believes that the symptoms of depression are themselves the symptoms of learned helplessness. He goes on 4 to clarify that it is not the trauma as such that produces dysfunctional behavior, but not having control over trauma Behan and Hirshfield (1966) and Hirshfield and Behan (1963) asserted that many of the most perplexing and resistant examples of chronic disability within the context of industrial accidents leading to human injury were actually the late stages in a sequential process. These researchers have cited four key features of this process. 1. The accident process is preceded by the development of tension and stress, which can lead to feelings of inadequacy and depression. 2. Dependency/denial can then occur. This is an essential component to the accident process; it incorporates the individual’s sensitivity to perceptions of increased tension and stress, while experiencing reduced support and approval from family members and/or social support networks previously expected to be supportive and approving. 3. The individual adapts a previously unacceptable role equated with weakness and failure into an acceptable role as a disabled person. 4. The individual’s continued role as a disabled person is crystallized and stabilized, thus becoming a means of coping with life. This role is energized by the individual’s personality characteristics and reinforced by social and family support networks. Weinstein (1978) later extended "the accident process" suggested by Behan and Hirshfield (1963) and Hirshfield and Behan (1966). He outlined five major stages. 1. mm. Tension and stress precede the accident event leading to feelings of inadequacy and depression. This stress is the result of a combination of a vulnerable character and specific job and/or family related tension. 5 2. MW. Job related injury, illness, or other problems (difficult working environment, incompatibility) are seen as 'explanation and sanction for decline in competence and increased dysphoria‘. (p. 96) 3. WWW. Diagnosis, treatment, and failure to find a cure add to the explanatory events. 4. Hi i f i ll“ . There is a decreased expectation of improvement, increased dependency on family and agencies with increased defensiveness and anger when pushed. 5. W. There is a loss of all expectation of recovery and a new role as honorably disabled is adopted (Weinstein, 1978). The occurrence of a disability in industrial settings is a common and costly phenomenum. According to Wickersham (1983), Worker Disability Compensation benefits paid in 1979 totaled 11.9 billion dollars in the USA. In 1981, as a result of work injuries, there were 95 million lost hours per week or 2.3% of the workweek lost, at a cost of 70 billion dollars. Thirty-one million individuals were limited in daily activities because of chronic physical or mental disabilities resulting from work related injuries. (Wickersham, 1983). The Social Security Disability Insurance Program is the largest insurance program in the country. In 1981, for instance, 17 billion dollars were expended to 2.8 million disabled adult beneficiaries. These adult beneficiaries had been assessed as having functional limitations from impairments so severe as to hinder them from sustaining substantial gainful work activity and/or activities of daily living. Berkowitz (1985) estimated that 121.6 billion dollars were expended in 1982 to cover cash disability payments, (Social Security Disability, long term disability, private disability insurance, veteran’s pensions) medical care costs, and direct services (vocational rehabilitation, veteran’s programs, and other 6 government sponsored programs). Berkowitz (1985) indicated that there are gaps in this information and therefore the actual costs are substantially higher. These costs do not reflect the cost that disability has on the family of the individual or the cost in production to the company. Adaptation to Stress f l r ' V r' According to Mechanic (1976). adaptation/adjustment to a stressful life event depends on whether the event itself presents a challenge mild enough to leave the person with coping resources relative to that individuals skills and capabilities. To determine such skills and abilities, Mechanic (1976) suggested that perceptual data be obtained. Locus of control perceptions have been investigated in several studies related to stressful life events. The locus of control concept emerged from the work of Rotter (1966) and his lntemal-External Locus of Control (l/E) Scale. This concept refers to the extent that individuals believe they have control over their lives as compared to those individuals who perceive that their lives are determined by forces outside themselves. Those individuals responding to the l/E Scale in such a manner as to indicate that they have control over their lives/life events are classified as being internal while those expressing a lack of control over their lives/life events are classified as being external. Locus of Control is also said to be descriptive of the degree to which individuals subscribe to the American individualistic orientation and/or the master of their fate perspectives (internal control). It is also thought to describe individuals who perceive themselves as “victims" of outside influences such as fate. luck, or chance (external control) (Hamlin, 1982). Locus of Control has also been found to be related significantly to certain measurable concepts (such as stress) and its effect on adaptation to the 7 stressful life event. For example, Mechanic (1976) conducted a study to examine the influence of locus of control orientations on individual responses to events considered aversive. It was hypothesized that locus of control was a stress moderator. Results indicated that when participants felt they could exercise some control over aversive events, the impact of stressors was decreased. Accordingly, 'extemals' were found to be less able to adapt/adjust to stress in a healthy manner. Thus the hypothesis was supported. A later study by Anderson (1977) used 90 entrepreneurs as subjects. The purpose of the research was to examine the relationship among several variables. Two variables were found to be of particular relevance. For example. correlations were calculated on stress and locus of control (using Rotter's Locus of Control Scale). Results showed a correlation (F- .61) at the .001 level of significance between external locus of control and stress. The subjects who scored high on the 'extemal" dimension, experienced greater stress. Anderson also found that 'externals" used more maladaptive or defensive types of coping behavior as compared to internals. A later study by Sandler and Lakey (1982) investigated stress moderating effects of locus of control beliefs. perceptions of control over negative events, and social supports. Results indicated that locus of control beliefs moderate the effects of stressors (i.e., the more internal an individual, the lesser the degree of anxiety.) Additionally, internals also appeared to have functioned with a smaller amount of social support than externals. While scholars have basically neglected the study of locus of control and the industrially disabled worker, Rosenbaum and Raz (1977) found some interesting results in an all-male, 70 person, Jewish sample. Of these, 26 suffered from locomotor disabilities while 44 other subjects were non-disabled. 8 The researchers had the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality lnventory’s Denial and Depression Subscale and the Locus of Control Survey adapted and translated into Hebrew (the study took place in Israel). Findings showed that: 1. The more serious the injury, the more the denial of that injury by the disabled subjects. 2. The disabled subjects reported significantly more symptoms of depression than the non-disabled subjects. 3. The more the subjects were found to be external, the more they believed that individual responses were not capable of changing the environment (although the means were still in the normal range). 4. External ratings on the Locus of Control measure may have been due to loss of reinforcers for internal type behavior and an increasing amount of punishers in the environment. According to the researchers, the environment/social order may have supported dependency behavior. There have been other studies examining locus of control and the disabled. Kundu (1983) developed a prediction model for vocational rehabilitation clients using locus of control, work motivation, work history and demographic variables. Although locus of control did not prove to be significantly related to work motivation, there was a significant relationship between locus of control and obtaining employment. lntemal subjects obtained employment significantly earlier than external subjects. Moreau (1983) investigated three different groups of disabled community college students (deaf, blind and orthopedically impaired) to able-bodied students. Her findings showed that physically disabled students were significame more external in their locus of control orientation than the able-bodied control groups. Overton (1987) examined locus of control as a 9 dispositional characteristic of physically disabled athletes. She found subjects to be more external than a group of able-bodied subjects. Furthermore, there were no gender differences in this sample on the locus of control measure. There is extensive research examining the locus of control construct. Kundu (1983) suggests that over 3,000 studies now exist in the literature utilizing this variable. Not only has this construct proven to be significant as a predictor variable but also as a moderator variable in the stress-adjustment process. Adaptation to Stress S'IS I III I' II'II Much attention has been paid to the degree to which the existence of social support can moderate the effects of stressful life events. With a breakdown of the family unit and its social support system, a proliferation of publicly supported and privately financed services have focused on replacing that support with professionals from various disciplines. There is wide disagreement in the literature about the definition of what constitutes social support. The literature on social support does not present a unitary concept. House (1985) suggested that social support involves a flow of one or more of four things between people: 1. W (empathy, concern, caring). 2. Instrumentalij (giving money. assistance). 3. Information (advice. suggestions, directions). 4. Appraisal (feedback or social comparison relevant to a person’s self evaluation). Regardless of definition. however, a consistent relationship has been found between individual physical and/or psychological health and social support. These findings tend to be most pronounced when compared to 10 individuals who are socially isolated and have had a breakdown in physical and/or psychological functioning. Gottleib (1981, 1985) has hypothesized that individuals experiencing difficulties that impair functioning, initially turn.to a personal community. This personal community can be likened to a primary social support group. The individual experiencing the trouble tends to go to such a community to get feedback and/or practical assistance to remedy the distressing situation. This personal community or primary group may have already detected that something was amiss with the individual with/without admission or verbal introduction (of the stressful life event). In either case, should the situation warrant it, a wider social support system may be mobilized to help the individual cope with the cause of the stress. Widely recognized among health and mental health scholars and professionals is the reality that, in general, those clients in need of such assistance delay treatment or defer help seeking behavior based on the following variables: 1. lack of motivation; 2. tendency to extemalize problems; 3. services are seen as too expensive, too professional, too bureaucratic. etc.; 4. self-reliance beliefs; 5. the stigma attached to the use of psychiatric services (Gottleib. 1985). Generally, however, individuals tend not to feel the same degree of hesitancy regarding the utilization of social support when there is a felt need. This reality fits well with the recognition that the first helpers experienced by most individuals were intimates from the family or friendship circle. Even with the breakdown of the extended family and the advent of the geographically 1 1 mobile individual (which tends to put miles between families and friends), in times of stress most individuals still turn to their social support system for aid, advice. and counsel (Crotty & Kulys. 1985). Social support research has generally heralded the efficiency and effectiveness of such support in mitigating the effects of stress/stressful life events on individuals’ physical and mental health. Cobb (1976) stated that among persons exposed to high stress, those with social supports of various kinds (friends, spouses, or community relationships) have much lower levels of symptomatology. compared with those lacking social supports. This relationship, Cobb (1976) further stated, is due in part to the fact that the supporting persons offer reassurance and acceptance of the stressed person and convey information which leads the subject to believe that he is esteemed and valued. Research by Myers, Linenthal, and Pepper (1975) has demonstrated a main effect for social support. Even under low stress. persons without social support show elevated rates of symptomatology. suggesting that the condition of low social support is itself a source of stress. Wills and Langner (1980) suggested that the combination of predictability and reassurance makes social support a potent insulator against stressful conditions. Dean and Lin (1977) examined the nature and significance of social support systems and attempted to clarify methodological and theoretical problems. In addition to suggesting possible approaches to measurement and design concepts, that would better delineate and clarify these sources. they contend that increased knowledge about the stress-buffering functions of social support has important implications for primary prevention. 12 Adaptation to Stress n r M r 'n V ' Verbrugge (1985) in her review of gender and health indicated that sex differences in health are principally the outcome of differential risks acquired from roles, stress, life styles, and preventative health practices among men and women in our society. Several studies dealing with gender and stressful life events suggest that under similar circumstances males and females react and adjust differently to stressful life events. Bloom and Caldwell (1981) examined sex differences in personal adjustment surrounding the time of marital separation. They concluded that during the preseparation period, women reported substantially poorer psychological adjustment than men. In contrast, during the postseparation period, men reported substantially poorer adjustment than women. Levenson. Hirschfield. Hirschfield, and Dzubay (1983) examined recent life events and the occurrence of industrial injuries with regard to gender differences. Their study on pro/post changes among White, Protestant. blue collar male and female workers involved in industrial injuries uncovered some interesting information. During the pre-injury period, males and females had experienced significant changes in life events and a higher degree of overall stress such that life change adjustments were at the major crisis level. Females had shown an increase in stressful life events W prior to the injury. Meanwhile. males experienced an increase in stressful life events gm prior to the injury. During this same period (i.e., the one year pro-injury period) while male stress was increasing, female stress was stable. During the post-injury period, females experienced more spousal separation 13 and more deaths of close friends. The female response was to express more independent behavior (such as achieving more education, making job changes. etc.). However, this may have been confounded with the need for additional income due to spousal separation. Meanwhile, these life events were predictive of diseases of adaptation (such as depression) in that these changes demand a psychological adaptation/adjustment. Study males reported increased hospitalization rates and more income than the study females. Study females earned only 60% of the wages reported by males. Also, low income status on the part of the study females meant that at the post-injury period of one year they had been disproportionately exposed to stressful life changes due to financial hardships. This finding has also been supported by Krause (1985). For example, Krause offers that social support networks, income status. and gender are moderators of stressful life events to the extent that they produce statistically significant interaction effects. A finding in the study by Levenson and colleagues (1983) that males showed more dependent behavior than females is contrary to that of Mechanic (1976). Using an unspecified sample of college students seeking assistance for psychological distress, Mechanic examined the degree of that distress and sociocultural variables. It was found that: 1. women show a longer duration of illness and more disability when compared to men; 2. women are more willing to report illness-related symptoms, admit to problems, and complain more openly than men; 3. men tend to deal more heavily in denial of illness symptomatology and have less physician contact than women; 4. men use less medication than women (with particular regard to 14 psychoactive drugs); 5. men tend to express distress in terms of aggressiveness, violence. smoking, and alcohol or drug abuse. Mechanic (1976) contended that these findings support the nofion that Western culture permits women to display illness and dependent behavior more than men. Men, Mechanic (1976) asserted, are expected to act and be more stoic and complain to a lesser degree than women. He also offers that because of gender specific social roles. women may be better able to accept illness at a lesser social cost and seek more solace than their gender counterparts. In summary. these findings also tend to hold for females and males regardless of age, marital status. or parental status. WWW: Research literature consistently suggests that stress buffering variables may prove to be of great importance in the disability process. Yet research suggests that these variables are interactive. Previous studies have emphasized examining each variable separately (Krause & Stryker, 1984, Lefcourt, 1981). Only recently has there been interest in exploring the possible interaction of these variables with each other. Caldwell, Pearson and Chin (1984) examined social support, gender, and locus of control within a college population. They found that gender was an important variable. Moreover, gender and locus of control orientation dramatically influenced social support and stress. They did not find an interaction between locus of control and stress while accounting for adjustment. In a study by Etzion (1984), work support was shown to be a stress buffering variable, but men and women used different sources (i.e. friends, family, etc). Etzion suggested that special attention should be given to the target population, the sources of support, and gender differences. 15 Clarification of what constitutes a stress moderator, stress mediator. stress buffer, and interaction of variables was discussed by Wheaton (1985). He reviewed stress research and suggested models for examining stress buffering functions. In his review, Wheaton stated that stress buffering involves the interaction between some potential source of stress and some factor in coping so as to define the conditions under which stress does and does not have an impact. He goes on to explain that the total casual effect of stress buffering is the sum of direct and indirect effects of stress through intervening variables. The issue of a stress moderator is further clarified by Wheaton. A resource that modifies the effect of stress (interactive version) points to a condition, or set of conditions, under which stress has substantially less impact. A variable that mediates (suppresses) the effect of stress (additive version) is generally mobilized by an increase in stress and as a result dampens its overall causal impact (p. 354). Wheaton (1985) suggested that certain variables may very well have dual buffering effects, that is, not only moderate, but also suppress the effects of stress on adaptation. He goes on to indicate that both are important and should be examined simultaneously to have a complete model. It is important to examine the main effects of variables as they may or may not impact upon stress. but it is also paramount that close scrutiny be given to interaction of variables with each other as it may have a more powerful effect on the total model. An interaction is important because it qualifies the interpretation of main effects. It allows the researcher to specify the conditions under which the variables have an effect. It may enable improved prediction of the outcomes of exposure to stressors and it may further our understanding of the process by which people adapt to stress. 16 W According to Tuck (1983) the majority of available literature on industrially injured workers does not describe the complicated psychosocial dynamics which accompany this stressful life event. Further scientific research is needed to investigate this population and to provide effective intervention strategies. While a great deal of research on locus of control orientation. adaptation/maladaption to stress, social support and gender differences has been reported, there has been a limited amount of scholarly research regarding the degree to which these variables can buffer the effect of stress/disability on the mental health of the industrially injured worker. Consequently, the purpose of this study was to examine these variables as they do or do not moderate the industrially injured worker’s adaptation to their disability, as well as their interaction with each other. It was proposed that this study would add to the scholarly base of knowledge in this area. It was expected that the results would be of interest to industrial researchers, rehabilitation counselors, and mental health professionals concerned with primary or secondary prevention of chronic disability, as well as government agencies dealing with this population. W When a researcher is examining four variables with several possible dimensions (e.g., social support: source or satisfaction) as well as two outcome variables, one can imagine the possible combinations that could be formulated. Therefore, this researcher has chosen to test those hypotheses that (a) appear to be supported by the literature, and (b) are of interest to the researcher. Based on the work of Rosenbaum and Raz (1977), Mechanic (1976), Anderson (1977), as well as Sandler and Lakey (1982), there is substantial evidence to 17 show that individuals who have external locus of control orientations will have greater experienced stress and will have higher levels of depression with more inability to cope, as opposed to internals, who will have lower experienced stress with lower levels of depression and higher coping ability. It was hypothesized that there is a moderating effect of locus of control orientation with stress level in the context of depression and inability to cope. Specifically an individual who is internal (as measured by the Rotter Scale) will have lower experienced stress (as measured by the Schedule of Recent Events) with lower depression (as measured by the Beck Depression Scale), and have a higher coping score (as measured by the Coping with Disability Inventory). This researcher examined by means of multiple regression the relationship of stress level, locus of control orientation and adjustment outcomes. It was further hypothesized that there is a significant main effect of locus of control orientation on the outcome variables as well as for stress level, and a significant interaction effect between locus of control orientation and stress in the context of the outcome variables (level of depression and level of coping). The following equation illustrates the main effect and interaction of these variables: Equation #1 D=a+b1 S+b2 LOC+b3 (S)d.OC) where D=score on the Beck Depression Scale a=constant S=stress as measured by the score on the SRE LOC=score on the Rotter Scale Equation #2 C=a+b1 S+b2 LOC+b3 (SxLOC) where C=score on the Coping with Disability Inventory (CDI) 18 Wyndham W1. Extemality will be directly related to stress level. 113291915112. Extemality will be inversely related to coping level. W. Extemality will be directly related to depression. MW. Locus of control moderates the effects of stress on depression and coping. The literature supporting the position that social support is a moderator of stress in the context of depression and coping is complicated in that one can examine the functional dimension of social support (i.e., sources of support) or the structural dimension (i.e., size, satisfaction). This researcher examined several studies that support the functional dimension of social support in moderating the effects of stress within the context of depression and coping. Etzion (1984) found that social support was a moderator of stress and that more importantly different sources of support were shown to be more significant than others. Caldwell et al. (1984) found similar findings. It was hypothesized that individuals who have high social support will experience less stress and therefore will have less depression and higher coping ability. Social support has a main effect as well as a significant interaction effect with the stress level in the context of coping and depression. Similar equations as previously shown demonstrate the main effect and interaction of a variable with stress on depression and coping. W W. Social support will be inversely related to stress level. M5116. Social support will be directly related to coping level. M95151. Social support will be inversely related to depression. W. Social support moderates the effects of stress on depression and coping. 19 Research examining gender and stress indicates that under similar circumstances males and females react and adjust differently to stressful life events. Studies by Levenson, Hirshfield, Hirshfield, and Dzubay (1983), Bloom and Caldwell (1981), and Krause (1985) as well as Etzion (1984) support the position that gender is a moderator of stress in the context of depression and coping. Based on the aforementioned research, this researcher hypothesized that females will have higher levels of stress two years prior to their injury. but have less depression and higher coping ability in the present. Men will experience higher stress one year prior to their injury. and have higher depression and lower coping ability in the present. There will be an interaction effect of gender with stress in the context of depression and coping. Again, equations demonstrating this relationship are similar to those previously shown for locus of control and stress with regard to depression and coping. W W. Females will experience higher levels of stress two years prior to their injury as compared to males. W. Males will experience higher levels of stress one year prior to their injury as compared to females. flypgtnesjsll. Females will experience less depression than males at the time of the study. W2. Females will have higher coping than males at the time of the study. 11mm. Gender moderates the effects of stress on depression and coping. 111mm. Females will have more social support in total than 20 males. There is substantial research to indicate that there is a significant interaction of the moderator variables with each other. The interaction of gender. social support and stress has been demonstrated in at least three studies: Caldwell et al. (1984), Etzion (1984) and Krause (1985). This researcher proposed that these phenomena may be due to gender differences in the socialization process found in our culture. Specifically, the position promoted by Gilligan (1982) would indicate that males and females must deal differently with the issues of autonomy and separation. These issues first come into play with the primary care-giver, usually the mother. While males must renounce their identification with the mother in order to identify with maleness, females do not. They can cultivate the nurturing qualities of the mother. As a result females see themselves in relationships or connected to others as opposed to males who have had to become autonomous and separate to claim their male identity. Obviously this is an oversimplification of the development process, but one can begin to see that relationships for males and females are different at a very early age and continue to flavor the individual's ability to develop relationships over the life span. This dual model of development leads the male towards independence/autonomy and the female towards interdependence/attachment. Work by Kahn and Antonucci (1980) as well as Tesch (1983) reviewed the concepts of role, social support, and friendship over the life span examining this aspect of development. With this theoretical background. this researcher proposed that there are gender differences with a significant interaction effect regarding social support and stress as it affects coping ability and depression. 21 mm W. Locus of control, social support and gender have a significant interaction effect when predicting coping and depression. William M15154. Extemality will be directly related to stress level. Hypothesis}. Extemality will be inversely related to coping level. W53. Extemality will be directly related to depression. 1131911135113. Locus of control moderates the effects of stress on depression and coping. W33. Social support will be inversely related to stress level. W. Social support will be directly related to coping level. W11. Social support will be inversely related to depression. W. Social support moderates the effects of stress on depression and coping. W19. Females will experience higher levels of stress two years prior to their injury as compared to males. W. Males will experience higher levels of stress one year prior to their injury as compared to females. W- Females will experience less depression than males at the time of the study. W12. Females will have higher coping than males at the time of the study. W- Gender moderates the effects of stress on depression and coping. MM. Females will have more social support in total than males. W. Locus of control, social support and gender have a 22 significant interaction effect when predicting coping and depression. Research—Mold It is apparent that there is a relationship between stressful life events and adverse changes in health. The question to be examined is what are the dynamics of this process and what factors play important roles. According to Gentry and Kobasa (1984) issues of examining stress and illness can no longer be examined in simple, unidimensional terms, but must take into consideration a second set of psychosocial factors - those that serve to buffer or cushion the impact of stress. Dohrenwend and Dohrenwend (1981) describe several hypotheses that could account for the life-stress process. The vulnerability hypothesis (See Figure 1) indicates that preexisting personal dispositions and social conditions of an individual moderate stressful life events which can then lead to adverse effects. The research model used in this study will add a third dimension: biological factors. specifically gender (See Figure 2). Specifically, a stressful life event (industrial injury/disability) is moderated by: 1. biological factors (i.e., sex); 2. personal disposition (i.e. intemal/extemal locus of control); 3. social situation (i.e., social support) which leads to adverse health changes (i.e., adaptation/depression). W This chapter addressed the need for a multidimensional stress research model that encompassed psychosocial factors as well as biological in examining the industrial disability process. The benefit of such a model is to better understand the complex experience which injured workers encounter as they they adjust and cope with their disabilities. A review of the literature including research on locus of control, social support, and gender revealed that 23 Figure 1 Vulnerability Hypothesis socrnr srrunmms srnrssrur I nnurnsr LIFE HEHLTH EUENTS runners PEBSONHL [I I SPOS ITI [INS 24 Figure 2 Modified Vulnerability Hypothesis SOCIHL SITOHTION: BIOLOGICHL SEN/HOE SOCIOL SUPPORT rfliisussrt’is- nnumsa ' HEHLTH cnnNers: INOUSTHIHL annrmrron PERSONHL / OISPOS ITI ONS: INTERNHUEHTEHNHL LOCUS OF CONTROL 25 these variables can play an important role in the stress-adjustment/coping process. In Chapter II methodology is presented. Included in this chapter is a discussion of the subjects in this study, sampling, as well as a review of the instrumentation and procedures utilized. Chapter III examines the statistical results of the research including sample characteristics on the variables of importance in the study. A discussion of the results, limitations of the study. as well the conclusions and implications for future research compromise the contents of Chapter IV. Chapter II MBLQQQIQQAL MM!) The previous chapter explored the need for a stress model to examine industrial injury as a stressful life event. Variables that have an impact on the stress-adjustment process were examined. Theory and research on internal-external locus of control, social support and gender have been reviewed. In this chapter a description of the subjects is given, along with the sampling procedures. Site and interview environment, instrumentation and data collection procedures then follow. Subject: Subjects for this study were 56 individuals (37 males and 19 females) who had experienced a work-related injury within the past 6-24 months, and were receiving voluntary Michigan Worker's Disability Compensation administered by Yeager and Company. The participants in this study ranged from 19 to 69 years of age. The average being 43 years of age (X=45, S.D.=12.89). Most members of this study were married (59%). Twenty-seven percent were divorced or widowed, while fourteen percent were single and had never married. (See Figure 3) The average education level for this group was 12.3 years while the range extended from 6th grade to Master’s level. (See Table 1). In terms of employment. 34% worked in the skilled trades prior to injury, 18% were factory workers. 14% worked for the govemment (municipalities), 10% were employed as service workers. (See Table 2). Ninety-six percent of the subjects were American born. Forty-two 26 27 I. Efingle I Divorced or Widowed I Married living with spouse Figure 3. Marital status of industrially injured workers participating in the study 28 Table 1 'ni 'll'n'r wrk in n=56 Education Frequency Percent (Highest level attained) 6 1 1.8 7 1 1 8 8 3 5.4 9 3 5.4 10 3 5.4 1 1 2 3.6 12 22 39.3 13 6 10.7 14 5 8.9 15 3 5.4 16 3 5.4 17 3 5.4 18 1 1.8 56 100.0 29 Factory Worker Clerical Worker Managerial Service Worker Skilled Trade Government Worker Other 56 Percent 17.9 1.8 5.4 10.7 33.9 14.3 16.1 100.0 30 subjects were White, 12 Black and 2 Hispanic (See Figure 4). I Hispanic I Black I White Figure 4. Race distribution of industrially injured workers in the study. The most frequent income bracket at the time of the study was $0-$4,999. This reflects income not including disability amounts. Twelve and one-half percent had income in the $20,000 to $24,999 bracket (See Table 3). W The pool of subjects for this study was referred by Yeager and Company, as administrator to self-insured employers in the State of Michigan for Worker's Compensation Disability Insurance. Yeager and Company indicated a willingness to cooperate with the principal investigator specifically, and the research team in general, during the course of the study. Claims personnel were briefed on the nature and purpose of the study and instructed to refer subjects to the researcher as part of an initial rehabilitation evaluation. The subjects were selected based on the following criteria: 1) they met the requirements established for disability status and were 31 Table 3 I II I rk Income Level Frequency Percent $04,999 26 46.4 $5.000-9.999 6 10.7 $10,000-14,999 3 5.4 $15,000-19,999 5 8.9 $20,000-24.999 7 12.5 $30.000-34.999 4 7.1 $35,000 and above 5 8.9 56 100.0 32 disabled within the last 6-24 months due to a work-related injury; 2) were currently disabled due to a work-related injury and were receiving disability benefits or payments through their employers via Yeager & Comany: 3) were mobile enough to report to the interview site; 4) appeared not to be experiencing memory problems regarding previously occurring dates and events, etc. and verify the same; 5) were willing to participate in the study and complete all study tasks as affinned by signing a Consent to Participate Form (See Appendix A). i in The setting for this research study was The Genesis Group in Southfield, Michigan. The Genesis Group is a rehabilitation facility which specializes in the rehabilitation of industrially injured and closed head injured clients. The Southfield office has ample facilities conducive to the maintenance of privacy and confidentiality within an interview situation, thus making it ideal for a study of this nature. The interviewing commenced on June 1, 1987. By September of the same year it became apparent to the researcher that referrals were slowing. The researcher met with the referral source to discuss the situation. It was revealed that there would be difficulty in referring as large a subject pool as originally proposed (100). Yeager agreed to allow the researcher to request referrals from the Grand Rapids and Saginaw offices. Additional subjects were obtained. The total secured was 56. Due to a variety of factors including time, cost/benefit. travel, and availability of subjects. no further attempts were made after December 31, 1987. 33 Amaratustlntam An interview area with two rooms conducive to the interviewing of potential subjects in an environment suggestive of privacy and confidentiality was utilized. Each of the two interview areas contained at least two chairs (one each for the interviewer and the client/subject). In addition, the interview area also contained a desk for the subject to record questionnaire item responses and a telephone. The room was well lighted and maintained a temperature that was seasonally comfortable. A supply of sharpened pencils with erasers was also readily available to the subject. In addition, it should be noted that several subjects were not feeling up to completing the form and asked to mail it back. After the initial intake, they were excused and given stamped envelopes to return the questionnaire (n=3, completed and returned questionnaire). lemma The seven instruments utilized during the study period are listed below. With the exception of the logbook, which was recorded and maintained by the research team throughout the study, all instruments were administered to the subjects/respondents. These instruments were completed in one interview session of approximately two hours duration. All subjects filled out a questionnaire packet consisting of the consent form and five scales. The Stressful Life Events Questionnaire was administered after consent was secured and the other scales were presented in four random orders. 1) ngbggls - A logbook was maintained by the research team. The purpose of the logbook was to record happenings, history. events. facts. etc., that may have influenced research outcome. Although the logbook cannot be measured quantitatively, it does reflect information regarding the cooperativeness of the subjects. (See Appendix G.) 34 2) WW (CPF) - A CPF was signed by all subjects as validation that they understood tasks related to the study. The CPF represents a virtual composite of research 'consent' forms. However, it was specifically adapted for use with industrially disabled workers (See Appendix A). The form also guaranteed confidentiality and that there was no potential for harm as a result of study participation. 3) WWW (SLEQ) - SUbIOCtS' life SVBSS was measured using the Schedule of Recent Experiences (Holmes & Rahe, 1967, Rahe. 1975). This 55—item questionnaire purports to examine stressful life events across five major categories: health. work, home and family, personal-social, and financial events that may have been experienced by respondents within the last 24 months. Rahe reports reliability correlations between .87 and .90 on test and retest for the Schedule of Recent Experience. On validity studies. correlations ranging between .50 and .75 were reported. Rahe (1975) found that when the questionnaire was followed up by personal interview, questionnaire errors were discovered which were usually in the direction of under-reporting on the part of the subject. (See Appendix B.) The questionnaire is designed to be self-administered. Respondents were to check those stressful life events that may have occurred to them. They were also asked to indicate the time frame (within the last 24 months) that the stressful life event occurred. A total score was obtained by adding all of the items checked. In addition. the questionnaire took into account the stressful life event in relationship to the injury. The number of events occurring after the injury, within the year prior to injury. etc. was obtained. Wan were obtained from this questionnaire to achieve a 35 profile of respondents/subjects. The researcher added demographic variables to the beginning/end of the SLEO to assess socioeconomic status (i.e., race, sex, education, income. occupation). Other demographics were those regarding the nunber of dependent children living in/out of the respondent’s household, number of years in present occupational position, origin of birth, etc. These items were specifically added to this questionnaire since it is the only instrument which was checked by the research team to minimize the effects of error in memory/under-reporting, as suggested by Rahe (1975). Demographic data were checked at the same time. Particularly sensitive items (such as race and income) were added to the end of the questionnaire. It was felt that respondents would have time to examine all survey items and build up a higher element of trust by the time they came to these items. 4) WI: (IIE Scale) - The Rotter (1966) Locus of Control Scale is commonly referred to as the l/E Scale. It is a 29-item forced choice questionnaire with six filler items. It was selected as me most accurate measure for this variable given its wide usage and acceptance in the literature. (See Appendix C) The Locus of Control Scale claims to measure individual beliefs across a wide range of situations regarding generalized expectations that one can control the outcome of certain situations. Therefore. the l/E Scale pays particular attention to dispositional influences (intemality/extemality) affected by past situations/behavior/experiences as they influence responses to current situational cues. 'lntemals' are those who expect or believe they have the power to affect situational outcomes (thereby positively influencing their ability to perform), and 'externals" are those who do not expect or believe they have such power (to 36 control outcomes). The scale is scored in the external direction (i.e., the higher the score, the more external the individual). The scale is designed to be self-administered. The total score is obtained by adding all questions answered in the external mode. Scores range from 0-23. Rotter (1966) reported internal consistency of .65 to .79. Reliability studies reported a range varying from .49 to .83. 5) W (303) - The Beck Depression Scale is an instrument that measures the degree to which respondents have depressive symptoms. The BDS contains 21 item sets. Each item set contains four possible choices of statements designed to reflect how the respondents have been feeling during the past month. The higher the item value selected within the set (on a scale of 0-3). the higher the depression (Beck, 1967). The BDS scores range from 0-63. The 808 was selected for its high reliability use in previous stress research (Sandler 8. Lakey, 1982; Caldwell, Pearson, & Chin, 1984). (See Appendix D.) Beck et al. (1988) reviewed research studies focusing on the psychometric properties of the BDI for the years 1961 through June, 1986. lntemal consistency yielded a coefficient alpha of .81 for nonpsychiatric subjects. With respect to test-retest reliability the BDl's correlations are greater than .60. Concurrent validity of the BDI with respect to other measures of depression is high as well. 6) WW (SSDS) - The SSDS is a new instrument utilized in life stress events/social support research, (Caldwell & Reinhart, 1988). This scale is designed to be self-administered. It claims to measure the amount of satisfaction an individual reports from four different support network sources including same and opposite sex friends, family, and helping 37 professionals. It also examines different kinds of support including emotional, practical, informational, advisory, and companionship. With permission from the author, four sources of support were adapted for this study: friends. co-workers, family, and professionals. Thus the SSDS focuses on specific kinds of support obtained from those in one’s social network. the general satisfaction with the various sources of support, how much support was received, and those individuals that comprise the network itself. (See Appendix E). Total support is obtained by adding items checked in each category (i.e., source, type of support). 7) Won (CDI) - The CDI is a newly developed instrument which measures coping behavior of physically disabled persons. The inventory is an 80 item, self-administered instrument. Subjects describe their preference on a five point scale ranging from never/rarely to almost always regarding various activities and statements of feelings toward themselves and others. Kulchami and Blom (1985) reported an alpha of .89 for the total coping score with a sample of 46 adult American individuals with disabilities involved in centers for independent living. The CDI has two subscales; the Process Coping subscale and the Outcome Subscale. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .79 and .84 respectively. Overton (1987) used the CDI on a group of 197 disabled athletes. The results of the reliability analyses on the CDI were supportive of the Total Scale score as a consistent measure of coping. A coefficient alpha of .89 and item-total correlations in the .40 and above range were reported. Kulchami (1985) reported for his sample a S.D.=27.25. The cut-off scores for low, medium, and high coping were 255. 283, and 311 respectively. 38 W The research team was comprised of two members. The author was the principal investigator (PI), and assumed all major responsibilities for the study such as obtaining all necessary agreements from appropriate parties, training the other research team member, and interviewing study participants. In addition, the Pl collected, maintained, coded, and analyzed all data. The other member of the research team assisted the PI by conducting interviews with study subjects. ‘ Both the principal investigator and the other team member have Masters Degrees in rehabilitation counseling. In addition, both have had many years of experience interviewing and counseling disabled workers who have experienmd a work-related injury. Research team members conducted interviews at The Genesis Group site. Each research team member interviewed subjects in a closed interview area to maintain client/subject comfort as well as their privacy and confidentiality. Em The majority of clients came to the Genesis Group during the study period. They were to report to this office approximately six months after the disability procedures/process at that time to ensure ongoing disability status/benefits (although for some disabled workers this time period may have varied somewhat with regard to the seriousness of the disability, length of the recovery period, etc.). It was necessary for the researchers to travel to Grand Rapids and clients’ homes to administer the balance of the questionnaire packets. 39 W Clients were referred to a member of the research team in the interview area to undergo the initial rehabilitation interview. During this interview, the research team member explained to the client their rights and obligations under the Michigan Workers’ Disability Compensation Act as well as obtain necessary information to determine the client’s eligibility/ineligibility for participation in the study. P r I If the client did not meet the criteria for selection and/or was not willing to participate in the study, after the standard rehabilitation interview, the client left the area. If the client meet the criteria for selection and was willing to participate in the study, they then signed the Consent to Participate Form (See Appendix I). They were then given the set of questionnaires by research team memberfinterviewer. Five and one-third percent of the eligible subjects did not consent to participate in the study. Mill The interviewer explained the tasks associated with the study (completing all questionnaires/questionnaire items) to the subject. At this time the client/subject was also assured verbally by the interviewer that their confidentiality and privacy would be maintained and that their disability benefits/payments were in no way connected to the study. Any questions posed by the subject regarding these issues were answered during this period before the subject began to respond to the questionnaire. Mullen! The interviewer then instructed the subject as to how to complete each of the questionnaires. Questions posed by the subject regarding this task were answered by the interviewer. When it appeared the subject understood the 40 tasks so that accuracy of response appeared reasonably ensured. the interviewer informed the subject of their departure from the area. The departure of the interviewer at this time enabled the subject to answer all questionnaire/items in an environment suggestive of privacy and confidentiality. The interviewer, however, informed the subject prior to their departure that they would be accessible immediately by extension phone should any questions arise when the questionnaires were completed. Subjects were also told not to leave the building until the interviewer returned in order to complete the rehabilitation interview. WM After completing all questionnaires, the subject notified the interviewer by phone. (However, when a fifteen minute time period elapsed in which the interviewer was not summoned by the subject. the interviewer voluntarily returned to the interview area.) After the interviewer returned to the interview area they asked the subject if any questions/problems arose during the completion of the questionnaire. The interviewer noted and recorded all/any comments in a logbook for later assessment that day by the Pl. (If a consensusftrend of problems surfaced regarding the questionnaires/items, the Pl made took any necessary steps to ensure the ongoing lntemal consistency and operation of the study.) The interviewer took possession of the questionnaires at that time. He/she then conducted a gently probing interview with the subject in regard to the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire (See Appendix B). The reason was to further ensure accuracy/completeness of response to all items. Subjects were then thanked for their participation and cooperation by the interviewer. (Note: Additional data obtained in this manner were recorded by the interviewer and included in the total stress score.) 41 W These procedures were repeated until the pool of subjects was exhausted within a six month period. The interview and the completion of the questionnaires took approximately one hour each. ll ‘ ' I All data were collected by the principal investigator (PI) and the research team memberfinterviewer as it was obtained. However, the Pl collected all data at the end of each interview and maintained it in a secure place. The PI also scored and coded all data. Another member of the research team recalculated all data to insure accuracy. Chapter III RESULTS mm This was an exploratory study to assess those factors that buffer individuals in a stressful situation. Specifically, locus of control. social support and gender were evaluated in terms of subjects’ reported stress. depression and coping with industrial injury. Two major areas were addressed in the analysis of the data. The first area presented a descriptive summary of sample characteristics on the variables of interest. These variables included the following: Locus of Control Scale, Coping with Disability Inventory, Beck Depression Scale, Social Support Dimension Scale, and the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire. The second area presented the formal testing of the hypotheses which were explored in the the study. The test statistics and analyses used in examining the hypotheses were Pearson Product-Moment Correlation, Multiple Regression and Host for independent samples. 9 = h-_ 1.11115 0.1!: 11.0.: o In : :1 in u: Ln In an effort to better understand the variables in this study, as well as to examine relationships that may present themselves. each variable is examined separately. Frequency data are provided for each of these variables. LmisctconlmLILQQl Locus of control was hypothesized to be significantly related to stress level as well as coping and depression. The mean LOC score for this sample was 10.0 (S.D.=3.5). The range extended from 3 to 17. There was no significant difference between male and female responses on this measure. Subjects were moderately external, based on Owen’s sample of able-bodied individuals as reported by Overton (1987), mean=8.3 (S.D.=3.9). The reliability 42 43 coefficient for this scale was .59. in ' h i 'I' I An individual’s ability to cope with stress especially from an industrial injury is an important aspect of the total disability process. The mean CDI score for this sample was 266.5 (S.D.=24.6), out of a possible 395. The range of individual scores for this sample was 188 to 315 indicating considerable variability among injured workers in coping with disability. There was no significant gender difference. 1= -.26, (df=54) p = n.s. A CDI score of 266.55 (Question #8 was omitted on the original questionnaire.) indicates that the subjects in this study were moderately coping with their disability. The reliability coefficient for this scale was .89. WHEEL) In this study subjects’ scores ranged from 0 to 41. The most frequent score was 8. The mean score was 9.13 (SD. = 6.49). According to Beck, the mean BDI scores for the minimal, mild, moderate. and severe classifications are 10.9 (SD. = 8.1), 18.7 (SD. = 10.2), 25.4 (SD. = 9.6) and 30.0 (SD. = 10.4), respectively (1967, p. 196). For this group, depression would be in the minimal range (9.13 < 10.9). There have been a number of studies examining the relationship of the BDI with gender. The overall pattern according to Beck (1988) has been mixed. Studies by Beck (1972), Nielson and Williams (1980) and Knight (1984) have reported that women have higher BDI mean scores than men. However, studies by Plumb and Holland (1977) and Schwab et al. (1967) have reported there was no significant relationship between gender and the BDI. There was no significant difference between male and female responses in this study. The reliability coefficient for this scale was .83 suggesting a high reliability value. 44 The Social Support Dimension Scale examines the functional aspects of social support. In this study four groups of individuals were examined: family, friends. co-workers, and helping professionals. Subjects checked the amount of support ranging from 0 to 3 (none, very little, average, and great) regarding four types of support (emotional support. practical assistance. companionship, and advice or information). The mean score for this group of subjects was 23.84 (S.D.=6.86). The range extended from 10 to 47. The most frequent score was 24. There were no significant differences between male and female responses on the total score or any of the sub-scores. The alpha value for this scale was .84. S! [III E I g I' . ISIEQI The total stress score for the period two years prior to injury ranged from 0 to 13. The mean was 2.96 (S.D.=3.35). The post (after injury) stress range was from 1 to 36. The mean was 9.23 (S.D.=7.38). In examining the stress scores one year prior to injury and the score for the period two years prior to injury, there is a remarkable increase in stress level in the period following the injury (6 months - 18 months). (See Table 4) The mean score for the combined time period of two years pre-injury and post-injury was 12.20 (S.D.=7.52). The reliability coefficient for this scale was .90. W Based on a review of the literature, individuals classified as external in their locus of control experience higher stress (Johnson & Sarason, 1978; Lefcourt, 1981). The Pearson product-moment correlation for locus of control and stress (with stress measured by the total number of events occurring 45 18912.4. l r Ti f ' ’n Mean Post injury 9.23” One year pre-injury 1.86“ Two years pro-injury 1.11” Life Incidence from birth to two years pre-injury 8.00‘ Two years pre-injury plus post-injury 12.20’ 7.37 2.53 2.11 6.66 7.56 Bangs 1-36 0-11 0-12 0-27 1 -36 'There were no significant differences between male and female responses on any of these variables. 46 between two years prior to injury and the time of the study on the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire) was 1=-.12, p> .10. Therefore. this hypothesis was not supported. W will invrlrl 'lv. Studies have been reported that would support the notion that individuals with external locus of control have lower coping ability (Kobasa, 1979; Sandler & Lakey, 1982). The Pearson product-moment correlation for locus of control and coping (measured by the total number of items checked on the Coping with Disability Inventory) was statistically significant _r=-.29, g< .05. Therefore, this hypothesis was supported indicating that external subjects had lower coping ability at the time of the study. HY I ll ir r l r ‘ n.The research literature lends support to the fact that individuals who are external will experience more depression (Sandler & Lakey, 1982; Mullen & Suls, 1981). A Pearson product-moment correlation for locus of control and depression (measured by the total score on the Beck Depression Scale) was statistically significant r=.34, p< .01. Therefore. this hypothesis was supported. HYEQTHESIS 4 . .10 M m: -. : h 3fi3'1 . 1 : 01 o.:- : ion 1n- maim- Previous research studies have supported the notion that locus of control moderates the effects of stress on depression (Johnson & Sarason, 1978; Kobasa. 1979; Sandler & Lakey. 1982). To test this hypothesis, two multiple regressions were computed. adding each variable separately. Stress was measured by the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire. The results show 47 13919.5 T I h r Mean SD. Bangs Locus of Control (LOC) 10.0 3.5 3-17 Coping with Disability (CDI) 266.5 24.6 188-315 Beck Depression Scale (BDS) 9.13 6.49 041 Social Support Dimension Scale (SSDS) 23.84 6.86 10—47 Stressful Life Events Post injury 9.23 7.37 1-36 One year pre-injury 1.86 2.53 0-11 Two years pro-injury 1.11 2.11 0-12 Life Incidence from birth to two years pre-injury 8.00 6.66 0-27 48 that locus of control does not moderate the effects of stress on depression. The same is true for coping. See Table 6. Therefore, this hypothesis is not supported. It should be noted that there was a statistically significant main effect of locus of control with stress in regard to depression and coping. W ' I 'll 'nv l v I. Work by Cassel (1974), Caplan (1974) and subsequent research (Caldwell & Bloom, 1982; LaRocco, House & F rench, 1980) all point to the fact that social support can mitigate the effects of stress. To test this hypothesis, a Pearson product-moment correlation was calculated between social support (measured by the Social Support Dimension Scale) and stress (measured by the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire). Looking at the pre-injury period one to two years prior to injury. the results were not statistically significant, p.13, g= n.s. The correlation for social support with stress pre and post injury combined revealed similar results, [=.09, p=n.s. Therefore. this hypothesis was not supported. i l 'l ir l i Literature by Caldwell and Bloom (1982), Etzion (1984). and Gottleib (1981), examine the relationship between social support and adjustment. The consensus is that social support is helpful in coping with stress. A Pearson product-moment correlation between social support and coping (measured by the Coping With Disability Inventory) revealed that [=.47, F000. which indicated that a statistically significant relationship existed. Closely related to the above hypothesis is the relationship between social support and depression. If one assumes that social support has a significant positive relationship with coping, one could conclude that the 49 m M In: ’:. :Ajmf. -0. . onu- ,'. e;.,; ‘m 11.0. .,.'n. QEEBESSLQN Variable 522m Sionmcame Stress (SLEO) .0550 .082 Locus of Control .1380 .004 Stress X Locus of Control .0379 .116 QQEINQ Stress (SLEO) .1112 .012 Locus of Control .0647 .046 Stress X Locus of Control .0025 .692 50 opposite would be true for social support and depression. W ' l 'l i The Pearson product-moment correlation between social support and depression as measured by the Beck Depression Scale was r--.19, p = .15. This relationship was not statistically significant. It appears that social support has a significant positive relationship with coping, but no significant relationship with depression. This hypothesis was, therefore. not supported. W .A. gnu 1.,q;.: : 3fi3°1.0f‘1(:‘e ._;.; i. .1. caning- Research on the moderating effects of social support is plentiful. Studies by Caldwell, Pearson and Chin (1987); Etzion (1984), Cohen and Wills (1985); and others have clarified and supported these phenomena. Two multiple regressions were calculated adding each variable separately. Stress was measured by the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire (SLEQ). There were no statistically significant interactions between social support/stress predicting depression or between social support/stress predicting coping. See Table 7. This hypothesis was not supported. It should be noted that there was a statistically significant main effect of social support on coping. W 3| 'I z ,-.:r: : u-h: lzv: . 1i : . t . :.. oor' - in: Research on injury and gender by Levenson et al. (1983) suggests that females experienced more stress than men two years prior to injury. During 51 marital separation, similar findings were noted (Bloom, Hodges, & Caldwell, 1982). In order to test this hypothesis. t-tests were computed between the stress levels of men and women during the specified time. A one-tailed t-test for independent samples was not statistically significant, 1=.26, (df-54) p > .05. The mean for males was 1.05. S.D.= 2.32 and the mean for females was 1.21, S.D.= 1.69. This hypothesis is not supported. W n. . _, -,;. :1. . u... -.vl . -1 m. . '. . 1. This hypothesis was tested by computing one-tailed t-test for independent samples. The result was not statistically significant, F.19, (df=54) Q > .05. The mean for males was 1.81, S.D.= 2.77 and the mean for females was 1.95. S.D.= 2.04. This hypothesis was not supported. W1 :11. . 'lI :103rlih : m-.r 'on- .n .. z .. l3 ' z . h study Studies examining gender differences in regard to depression have overwhelmingly shown that women in Western cultures have higher rates of affective and anxiety disorders than do men (Miller & Kirsch, 1987). However, recent investigations among college students and other young homogeneous adult populations have found equal rates of morbidity in males and females - at least for depression - thus casting doubt on the notion that females are universally more depressed than males (Jenkins, 1985). Clear explanations are not found to support these mixed findings. To test this hypothesis, the test of choice was a one-tailed t-test for independent samples. The result was not statistically significant. tr-.32, 'r - M I-o'l : R z or: llEEEflflSSflQN. lflufiflifi Stress (SLEO) Social Support Stress X Social Support SZQEOEE lflflfiflifi Stress (SLEQ) SochuSkuxxxt Stress X Social Support .oo'nfr YAL I .A "| 52 JPmAWII lz-r:.I-1 -. 0 HI 0 52m 31m .0550 .082 .0412 .126 .0264 .216 32.903009 $199M .1112 .012 .2133 .000 .0018 .713 53 (df=52.9) p > .05. The mean for males was 9.30, S.D.=7.4 and the mean for females was 8.79, S.D.=4.33. The depression level was not significantly different for men and women at the time of the study. Therefore, this hypothesis was not supported. Here again, research findings have been mixed. Depending on the particular aspect of c0ping or strategy used, studies have not been overwhelmingly significant regarding gender differences. According to Miller and Kirsh (1987), ”the available evidence shows that females are more prone to psychological distress than males are, yet do not consistently demonstrate any underlying cognitive characteristic that would account for this difference.” (p. 298). The t-test for independent samples revealed t=.26, (dh54), p > .05. There was no statistically significant difference between men and women in regard to coping at the time of the study. The mean for males was 267.14, S.D.=26.32 and the mean for females was 265.32. S.D.=21.70. Therefore, this hypothesis was not supported. W : -: mun. . .. : :mb . 1 z . 0.1 -_:.-.:.1-1 -_ .o. mil -. Research has shown that gender can play an important role in the stress-Illness process. Work by Caldwell and Bloom, (1982) as well as Levenson, Hirshfield, et al., (1983) support the notion that men and women experience and react differently to stress. and that gender moderates stress with regard to adjustment and coping. Multiple regression results showed that there was a statistically significant moderating effect of gender on the relationship between stress and 54 depression, but not between stress and coping. (See Table 8). Research on gender differences in regard to social support and stress are mixed. Belle’s review (1987) examined gender differences. As cited in Belle (1987), Miller and lngham (1976) found that adult men had larger networks than women while Weiss and Lowenthal (1975) discovered that the women in their sample had more friends than did men. But yet. Caldwell and Peplau (1982) found no differences between male and female unmarried college students in the total number of friends. To test this hypothesis, a one-tailed t-test for independent samples was performed. The results were (=62. (df=54), p > .05. The mean for males was 23.43. S.D.=7.37 and the mean for females was 24.63. S.D.=- 5.84. Thus this hypothesis was not supported, suggesting that for this sample there were no statistically significant differences between men and women in regard to social support at the time of the sturdy. There is substantial evidence that moderator variables interact with each other. (Caldwell et al., 1984; Etzion, 1984; Krause, 1985). Multiple regression analysis placing all variables in the equation, as well as all possible combinations, revealed no significant interactions in regard to coping. Stress was measured by the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire. With depression, there was a significant stress, locus of control, gender interaction. as well as a significant stress, social support, locus of control, gender interaction. (See Table 9.) Therefore, this hypothesis was supported. Discussion of this 55 tastes I' I r i nf r r ' i P E I N We 52mm: Siam Stress (SLEQ) .0550 .082 Sex .0013 .791 Stress X Sex .0789 .034 coarse Mariam 132$me sum Stress (SLEO) .1112 .012 Sex .0003 .899 Stress X Sex .0000 .980 Stress(SLEQ) Sex LocusofControl SocialSupport SXSSXLOC SXSSXSEX SXLOCXSEX SXSSXLOCXSEX SXLOC SXSS SXSEX 56 of 0 [0 32mm .0550 .0013 .1418 .0070 .2252 .0511 .0170 eee . e_ : e: Wit Si 'fi .082 .790 .255 .504 .056 .024 it it .627 Table 9 continues... Iagntialcontd) QQELNG Mariana Stress Sex LocusofControl SocialSupport SXSSXLOC SXSSXSEX SXLOCXSEX SXSSXLOCXSEX SXLOC SXSS SXSEX " Significant p < .05 " These variables were added in one block with S X SEX 57 .BgSZBHKEt .Skrflkamce .1112 .012 .0003 .899 .0661 .046 .1820 .001* .0100 .377 .0177 .239 .0128 .316 .0138 .298 .0068 .915 58 interaction is covered in the following chapter. 59 8822 Storm 6200.51 6052304. 886m Storm 8822 8882 .8882 Storm 8% 3882 289 oz 8.x a E98239 85 a oz wax u oz 50¢ a mo> mox a 62 6589 8.x a oz E23239 mod a oz mo.v a mo> mo.v a mo> mod n oz axe.— g £3.24. 378291 223.22 5:22.00 comuaon. 536.260 comcmoc consoLLoO conned m_m>_mc< commoaom 29.3.2 5:52.00 confine 5:22.00 cooked 3:22.00 c023,". lug Bowman“ >0. CM .12..- 14 .P _ mcfioo can connoaoo :0 £023 65 $2808 tendon Room c2823“. 2 6222 28.95 toads» Ecom .26. 9.68 2 6222 2.025 toaasm .mcom .o>o_ mocha 2 33.2 20262.: toocsm Boom 938 can cohesion co mocha moSmLoooE .obcoo 6 38.. cohesion 2 0222 282% £3.55 .90. 0:38 2 3.22 26295 £3an .32 82% 2 .222 826 3.355 m: NI m: m: I... m: NI PI 3% «Benson»: . Hi< >1444 V4 60 03033". ucaaooo< 08022". 03023. Ucficoo< 383m 883m 883m 886m 3882 8:39 35 a oz E23239 mov a mo> moK 0 oz 6:39 3.x a oz 3.3669 mox a 3> 8.x a oz 35 a oz 85 a 62 8.x a 62 .93... Eng 3335‘ 3.323.; 23.3,. “no?“ «322.4. 3323". 26.3.22 32... “on.“ $8.. «moi 18% 303235 .026 c2822... go: 323 can «.033 Econ .23 B 283 .59 E toaaam Econ 20:. 333“. @530 can 3.3236 co 32% «22000:. .2036 62m 6 0E: “a @530 3:9... mofiEo... saw 6 2... .6 3323—3 32 3.23". >5? 9 corn 3% oco 32% 3:9: «262 .995 2 Long 23» 92 323 3:9: 3.23". 3% m—I VFI «PI «2.. :I or: m: 1.33.21 £3503: Chapter IV mm This chapter will commence by discussing and interpreting the results. A critique and discussion of the limitations of the study follow. Finally, conclusions and implications for future research will be presented. W The purpose of this study was to better understand the complex psychosocial dynamics which accompany industrial injury and resulting disability. Specifically, a multidimensional model of stress and variables which affect adaptation to stress. was explored. The conceptual model utilized included social support, locus of control, and gender. There is growing support for including more than one type of moderator variable in order to examine possible interactions of these variables. as well as develop a strong, conceptual model (Caldwell et al., 1984, Dean & Ensel. 1982). The first hypothesis tested the relationship between the subjects” locus of control orientation as measured by the lnternal-Extemal Locus of Control Scale, and stress level as measured by the Schedule of Recent Experience. The LOC construct has been defined both as a situational construct as well as a dispositional construct. Lefcourt (1966) suggested that LOC can be conceptualized as a dispositional characteristic which refers to a broad range of situations in which an individual perceives having control over events happening to him or her. According to Srull and Karabenick (1975) LOG can be described as a situational variable in general. This construct is an indication of an individual’s generalized belief that event outcomes are contingent upon one's ability (internal) versus forces beyond control such as chance or fate (external). This instrument has been used extensively in research. It has been significant in predicting variables ranging from personal adjustment to job 61 62 satisfaction within various groups of subjects ranging from children to the elderly, as well psychiatric patients, minorities and the disabled. In this study the reliability coefficient for the LOC scale was .59. This coefficient reflects the extent to which a measure is free of error variance. When the coeffiecient is low as in this case, precaution must be taken in interpreting results. However, if an instrument has low reliability, yet demonstrates statistically significant correlations, one could argue that even under high error variance the relationship of the variables is strong. Nunnally (1978) stated that one of the most important uses of the reliability coefficient is in estimating the extent to which obtained correlations between variables are attentuated by measurement error. As the reliability increases. so does the correlation. Certainly, one would urge that future research utilize more reliable measures to test the relationships between variables in question. The Schedule of Recent Experience is one of the most highly utilized instruments in stress research. There have been numerous stress scales derived from this measure. Controversy regarding the use of weighted and unweighted items is still found in the literature. According to Shrout (1984) although there has been discussion in the literature on life-event scaling for more than a decade, there has been no conclusive proof that one method is better than another in predicting health status. To this date the perfect life stress questionnaire has not been discovered. The results of the first hypothesis showed no statistically significant relationship between externality on the locus of control measure and stress level. This finding is contrary to much of the research literature demonstrating a relationship between locus of control and life-event measures. Most findings in this regard point to the fact that externals experience more stress or report higher levels of stress (Kobasa, 1979, Sarason et al., 1978). However, Lefcourt 63 (1984) reported a relatively independent relationship between Rotter’s l-E scale and a life-event measure in a group of fifty-nine college students. His explanation for this phenomenum is that the life-events measure, which requests that a subject recall past events, may be biased by the moods that are assessed by the mood scales; and in turn, locus of control scores may likewise reflect current moods. Hypotheses two and three are closely related. A statistically significant negative relationship was established for hypothesis two between the subjects’ locus of control orientation as measured by the Internal-External Locus of Control Scale and coping level as measured by the Coping With Disability Inventory. In hypothesis three a statistically significant positive relationship was established between the subjects’ locus of control orientation as measured by the Internal-External Locus of Control Scale and depression as measured by the Beck Depression Scale. The CDI is a relatively new instrument. The original work by Kulchami and Blom (1985) was designed to assess coping status. defined as an individual’s current status with the use of coping behaviors and the level of achievement of coping outcomes (social competence and quality of life). Overton (1987) further analyzed the CDI. Her findings supported the validity and reliability of the total score, however, the validity of the subscales of this instrument, as originally proposed by Kulchami (1985) was not supported. She admits that her findings should be viewed with caution because her small homogeneous sample limits the generalizability of her results. Overton (1987) stated in her review of Kulcharni’s work (1985) that this scale separated the process of coping from the outcomes of coping. The process of COping included coping behaviors similar to the description by Lazarus and Folkman 64 (1985) but more specific to disability. The outcomes of coping were suggestive of increased quality of life and increased competence. The definition of coping according to Kulchami (1985) is: An active psychosocial process characterized by a persistent effort to overcome, master and solve problems, issues, and dilemmas within the person and in the outside world, connected to or unconnected with the disability. This process occurs in the context of individual-environmental transactions, such as belief systems and cultural practices. Coping facilitates the development of competence and quality of life, which are behavioral outcomes, but it does not guarantee their occurrence (p. 17). This researcher felt that the total score on the coping with disability inventory would by a good outcome measure to indicate an individual’s behavioral and psychological adjustment to injury. In regard to the Beck Depression Scale, twenty-five years of research literature about this instrument has accumulated since it’s introduction in 1961 (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock & Erbaugh, 1961). It was revised in 1971 and copyrighted in 1978 (Beck, Rush, Shaw & Emery, 1979). The Beck scale is one of the most popular and widely used instruments utilized in assessing the intensity of depression in psychiatrically diagnosed patients (Piotrowski, Sherry & Keller, 1985), but also for examining depression in nonpsychiatric populations (Steer, Beck & Garrison, 1986). In examining the results of hypotheses two and three. it would seem logical that if external subjects are less effective at coping, then they would also suffer higher levels of depression. For this group of subjects, this was the case. The research literature examining locus of control and coping/depression is fairly consistent. The earliest support for the control construct with depression is the work by Johnson and Sarason (1978). Their basic finding was that only those subjects with external locus of control 65 experienced depression in the face of stressful life events. In a somewhat related study by Huisaini, Neff, Newbrough and Moore (1982) in seven out of eight separate regression analyses, the subjects who had a sense of "personal competence' were less likely to experience depression when dealing with life events. Benassi, Sweeney and Dufour (1988) reported findings that the greater the externality of the subject, the greater the depression experienced. Locus of control was significantly related to depression. This relationship was moderately strong and consistent across the studies they reviewed. Hypothesis four stated that locus of control would moderate the effects of stress in the context of depression and coping. That is, there would be an interactive effect between locus of control and stress. The results did not bear this out. Cohen and Edward (1986) concluded that locus of control orientation is the personality characteristic with the most consistent and strongest evidence for stress buffering. In the present study there was a statistically significant main effect of locus of control with depression and coping. Some investigators equate evidence for stress buffering exclusively with a demonstrated interaction between stress and a variable (e.g., locus of control, social support) (Gore, 1981, Thoits, 1982), while others suggest an additive version of stress-buffering as well ( Billings and Moos, 1981, Dean 8. Lin, 1977). Wheaton (1985) clarifies this researcher’s position on this issue. He distinguishes between types of stress-buffering. A resource that moderates the effect of stress (interactive version) points to a condition, under which stress has substantially less impact. He goes on to explain that a resource that suppresses the effect of stress (main effect version) is generally mobilized by increases in stress, and as a result, dampens its overall causal impact. The suppressor role of a coping resource applies equally across all levels of stress. In the case of a moderator variable, 66 because of the multiplicative nature of the effect. the reduction in distress resulting from the resource typically will increase as stress increases. Another type of main effect variable is one that is independent of the level of stress. It is benficial to the indivdual under high or low stress. For example, locus of control is not mobilized (i.e., the higher the stress, the more internal) but rather has an independent effect under any stress level. Hypothesis five tested the relationship between the subjects’ level of social support as measured by the Social Smport Dimension Scale and level of stress as measured by the Schedule of Recent Experience. The Social Support Dimension Scale is a newly developed instrument (Caldwell & Reinhart, 1988). There has been limited usage of this measure, but reliability testing has demonstrated high alpha values. The results of this hypothesis were not statistically significant indicating that here was no relationship between social support and stress level. Three studies were found in the literature which specifically noted that their stress and support measures were not correlated (Habif & Lahey, Paykel et al., 1980, Sandler, 1980). This finding helped to rule out the possibility that buffering effects may be artifactual in light of significant buffering interactions in these studies. It should be noted that in the present study there was a statistically significant main effect of social support on coping. This finding covers the content of hypothesis six. The more support one has, the higher the coping level. Social support can provide resources such as reassurance of worth, assistance in problem solving, and practical assistance; these resources are critical to the development of self-concept and esteem, they help prevent demoralization in times of stress, they increase options when dealing with adaptation or loss, and they sometimes facilitate a more active style of coping (Antonucci 8. Depner, 1982; Cobb, 1976; Hirsch, 1981). 67 In hypothesis seven, the relationship between social support as measured by the Social Support Dimension Scale and depression as measured by the Beck Depression Scale was tested. No statistically significant relationship was established. This lack of a significant negative relationship between social support and depression is contrary to the majority of research examining these variables. One possible explanation may be the instrument utilized to measure the variables. At this juncture in the research literature, a dialogue continues regarding what constitutes support, and how it should be measured. The Social Support Dimension Scale is relatively new and it may not be sensitive to the particular aspect of social support that would demonstrate a significant relationship with depression. Another possible explanation could be the subjects” understanding of the scale as well as possible denial in regard to depression. This group of disabled workers was moderately coping and minimally depressed. Additionally, these phenomena could well be peculiar to this group of individuals. Social support did not have a statistically significant effect in regard to moderating the effects of stress in the context of depression and coping. That is, there was no interaction effect between stress and social support in the context of depression or coping. There was a statistically significant main effect of social support with coping but not with depression. In examining extensive research studies, Payne and Jones (1987) stated that the results are mixed regarding both the main effects as well as the interactive effects of social support. Several researchers (Thoits, 1982; Kessler et al., 1985) almost take for granted that evidence exists demonstrating a strong association between social support and psychological symptoms. However, Leavy (1983) does caution that the size of the association is weak. Payne and Jones (1987) in their review on main effects between social 68 support and psychological symptomatology argue that the range of variance shared between support and symptoms is O to 26% (Lin et al., 1979; Cohen et al., 1984). They further state that evidence in smport of the interaction effect is also not clear. One of the possible explanations why the evidence is not clear is that there may be measurement contamination. For example, a depressed person may report they get poor support because they are depressed. (The symptoms may affect the way they perceive the quality of support or the support they actually receive.) Hypotheses nine and ten examined stress levels one to two years prior to injury for males and females. No differences were found to be statistically significant for either time period. These findings are contrary to the results of Levenson et al., (1983) regarding sex differences in industrial injury as well as other research. The Levenson study (1983) had a much larger sample. This could possibly explain the difference in results. Also, the present study did not break down the sources of the stressors but utilized a total stress score. Perhaps closer examination of these sources would demonstrate differences between men and women. It is interesting to note that in the Levenson study (1983) there were no gender differences post injury regarding level of stress reported as was true in the present study. Hypotheses eleven and twelve are closely related and therefore, will be discussed in conjunction with each other. Hypothesis eleven tested for differences between male and females regarding depression as measured by the Beck Depression Scale, while hypothesis twelve tested for differences between males and females regarding coping as measured by the Coping with Disability Inventory. No statistically significant differences were established. According to AI-lssa (1982) women report significantly higher rates of 69 psychological distress than men. Other researchers have demonstrated that women adjust somewhat better than men to the death of a spouse (Stroebe 8 Stroebe, 1983), that they are less emotionally affected than men by financial difficulties (Kessler, 1982) and cope better than men with the long-term aftermath of separation and divorce (Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980). Miller and Kirsh (1987) have suggested several plausible explanations why the results are mixed regarding sex differences in the context of coping and depression: (1) past research has overlooked the most likely domains for the existence of these differences (is. males and females differ in their use of aggression; "acting out" may be a significant variable to consider as a potential mode for coping). (2) male and female responses may be biased in that males tend to underreport their symptoms, while females may overreport. It is possible that males may ’mask" their depression due to the socialization process. (3) traditional theories do not tap the relevant cognitions to explain the sex differences in depression and coping or the opposite, that the cognitive level is not the route for sex differences. This view suggests that perhaps the higher incidence of pathology in females may be due to some other etiological factor, such as higher incidence of life stresses, biological predisposition, etc. Hypothesis thirteen examined the possible interactive effects of gender and stress as measured by the Stressful Life Events Questionnaire in the context of coping and depression. There was a statistically significant interactive effect with gender and stress in the context of depression, but not with coping. In examining the particular conditions under which the interaction takes place, it would appear that under high stress women were significantly less depressed (See Table 11), the opposite was true under low stress. One possible explanation for this finding is that females have a diverse 7O T-.oz1' z-. =- or: f- 1 . F ._l: he: Stress Stress High Low Females 8.4 9.22 Males 10.79 7.72 Overall population mean = 9.12. Hi ._n- Low 71 set of roles, (i.e. mother, wife, worker) that require equal amounts of demands upon them, while men place a larger emphasis on work or being the bread winner. When work is removed from men’s lives it may cause more stress, consequently, they may feel more hopeless than females who may experience relief rather than depression. Hypothesis fourteen tested the gender differences in social support as measured by the Social Support Dimension Scale. No statistically significant difference was established. Again, the sample was small, and perhaps the items on the social support scale may not have been sensitive enough to detect any difference. The last hypothesis tested the interaction effect of gender locus of control and social support in the context of coping and depression. There were no interactions of variables in regard to coping. There was however a statistically significant interaction effect of locus of control, gender and stress with depression as well as one with gender. stress, locus of control and social support. Examination of these interactions are helpful. It would appear that internal women under high stress experienced the least amount of depression (See Table 12). External men on the other hand under high stress experienced the most depression. Internal locus of control seemed to produce the lowest depression across the board except for internal women under low stress. This might be explained in terms of the conceptualization of the life events included in the stress questionnaire. There has been research to indicate that daily disruptions or 'hassles‘ are more difficult for women than major life events (Delongis, Coyne, Dahof, Folkman & Lazarus, 1982). Another plausible explanation is given by Rotter (1966). He indicated that extreme intemality may work against adjustment and could indicate significant maladjustment. In terms of the interaction effect of stress with gender, locus of control, Females Hales 72 :I c-er: for l; :. e; :1... oc-_ 0 our. Stress High Low Locus of control Internal External Internal External 5.33 9.71 10.00 8.60 8.40 13.44 6.10 9.75 Entire population mean = 9.13 73 and social support, some interesting findings are noted, but difficult to explain. The highest depression mean occurred for external men under high stress with high support (see Table 13). The lowest depression level was for internal women under high stress with low social support. Women experienced generally lower depression except for two situations both with external locus of control, but in one instance stress was low with support high and in the other stress was high with support low. Because of the size of this sample, caution must be taken regarding interpretation of the above findings. For example. some of the cells have only one or two subjects. Regarding gender differences in locus of control and stress, research results are mixed. Toves, Schill, and Ramanaiah (1981), replicated the earliest control study by Johnson and Sarason (1978) and found internal locus of control to be a stress resistance resource to men only. Lefcourt (1982) described subdividing his subjects and findings that internal/external orientation had no impact on how women responded to stressors. The males in the study enjoyed the buffering effect. A third study by Huisaini et al. (1982) showed even more diverse findings. They concluded that for both men and women, there were direct influences from life events and personal competence on depression. In 1978 Pearlin and Schooner examined important data on women and control. One of their conclusions is that mastery (the extent to which one sees one’s life chances as being under one’s control in contrast to being fatalistically ruled) is found predominantly among the males in the study. They attribute this difference to socialization patterns which less adequately prepare women to deal with the stressors of life. There were no gender differences in the present study of industrially injured workers. Research by Overton (1987) demonstrated similar results with 74 01 0 1|. 0 . 1 ' Brzzlou n Dl A. for . -.:r o. W Stress High Low Social Support High Low High Low Locus of control Int: Ext Int Ext Int Ext Int Ext Females 6.00 10.66 4.00 9.00 8.50 7.00 11.5011.00 Hales 8.62 17.33 7.50 6.66 6.20 8.75 6.00 10.75 Entire Population Mean Score = 9.13 75 a group of disabled athletes. One of the possible explanations for this study’s results may be that employed men and women in jobs with similar levels of control and demand might be considered having similar roles. Additionally, when considering industrial injury, one could conclude that the experience is similar for boh sexes because of the unexpected nature of injury. Bamet, Biener and Baruch (1987) felt hat the task for researchers was to identify gender-relevant situations that are perceived as equally stressful and then examine stress reactions. This study utilized a correlational survey approach. According to Fox (1969), the correlational survey is designed to estimate the extent to which different variables are related to each other in the population of interest. The critical distinguishing characteristic is he effort to estimate a relationship. There are several advantages to his type of research: (a) it permits one to measure a great number of variables and their interrelationships simultaneously; (b) it provides information concerning the degree of relationships between he variables being studied; (c) it is appropriate when the research situation is not particularly suited to he experimental mehod. Although the correlational method does serve as a useful hypothesis-generating procedure, the results of such an investigation can never be statements of cause and effect such as is the case of true experiments, and therefore generalizations from his study are limited. Hypoheses for possible research consideration are included at he end of his section. A furher limitation of this study was he size of he sample. The referral source was not able to produce he number of subjects originally proposed. Attempts were made to increase he size, but due to time constraints, cost, and 76 location of potential subjects, efforts were discontinued after 56 subjects had been interviewed. These subjects all volunteered to participate. They were not randomly obtained, and most likely he results are not representative of he total population of disabled workers. Anoher drawback of his study was he lack of a control group so hat comparisons could be made between groups, in an attempt to discover significant differences and similarities. Beyond the limitations already mentioned, it is apparent that the instruments utilized required a great deal of time and persistence to complete. Many of hese subjects requested assistance with directions and/or reading of he material. Due to he nature of some of he injuries, several clients asked to complete the questionnaires at home because hey were experiencing pain or discomfort which affected heir concentration. Lastly, the instrumentation utilized had limitations. Overton (1987) completed extensive investigation on he Coping with Disability Inventory. She recommended hat his questionnaire required furher study to discover what it is measuring in more detail. She did feel hat here was validity and reliability for he total score as opposed to the subscales. The use of he Stressful Life Events Questionnaire had some limitations as well hat may have affected he accuracy and reliability of he total score. It is important to note that the demographic portion of the SLEO had never been used before, and it is possible hat he construction or wording biased responses from participants on these data. In addition here have been numerous studies concerning the nature of stressful life events. Specifically, research has focused on the question of wheher he stressfulness of an event is a function or he amount of change it entails, regardless of whether he change is positive or negative, or whether only events hat involve an undesirable change are stressful 77 (Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1984). Discussion and investigation into this question is still mixed. Perhaps, further refinement of stress questionnaires in general is necessary before a truly reliable measure can be established. Shrout (1984) has suggested hat future research utilizing life-event scaling should seriously consider weighting of he items. This is he only way to discover what it is about certain life events hat makes hem more related to illness han oher kinds of events. The difference among events is what is achieved by weighting. Anoher consideration is hat each new population under investigation may have important life events hat are peculiar to heir situation. It would be paramount to identify he characteristics of he important life events in order to capture he complete impact they might have upon the population under investigation. 9909105103.: It is evident hat he stress process is a complex one. This study examined industrial injury wihin he context of his process by taking into account variables that positively or negatively influenced the individual’s adjustment. This was accomplished by utilizing a multidimensional model, incorporating biological, social, and personality moderators (gender, social support and locus of control). It is important to re-exam he conceptual model developed for his study. Based on he results which were obtained from his research the following can be stated: ( 1) Gender was he only variable examined that had an interactive or modifier effect on he outcome variable of depression. In addition, here was a statistically significant interaction effect of the variables with each other in regard to depression. Specifically, here was a STRESS x GENDER x LOCUS 78 OF CONTROL interaction, as well as a STRESS x GENDER x LOCUS OF CONTROL x SOCIAL SUPPORT interaction. Recent work by Krause (1987) attempted to demonstrate hat social support operates by bolstering lntemal locus of control beliefs. This very well may explain why only one of he variables had an interaction effect singularly, but produced several when taken in combination. (2) A statistically significant main effect was demonstrated for locus of control on boh coping and depression. In a longitudinal study by Huisaini and von F rank, A. (1985), results showed hat locus of control, as a measure of personal resources, had he most marked effect on depression. (3) Social support demonshated a main effect on coping. (4) There were no gender differences on any of he variables. That is men and women experienced his life event in a similar manner. A recent article by Ell (1986) called for a more integrative conceptual framework to advance heory and enhance he applied value of research data. She suggested hat researchers include social support, locus of control, and various coping strategies in investigation. There are some practical implications that can be derived from he present study. If locus of control and social support have main effects on depression and coping, hen it would be reasonable to believe that injured workers need to feel hat hey have some control over heir lives and hat hey have support in order to cope and adjust to heir situation. It is also important to note that men and women have different reactions to stress. Perhaps fine tuning our understanding of hese dynamics will allow counselors, claims personnel, physicians, and ohers who interact wih this group, to assist more effectively. 79 lmli 'n rF rR r This research is only a preliminary study into the complex injury disability process. These are several characteristics of stress research which make his a challenging area of investigation in regard to workers’ disability. First of all, he basic goal of research is to establish cause-effect relationships rather han merely descriptive. It would be helpful to definitively understand hose variables hat assist or harm individuals in an injury situation. Obviously, he more understanding of his process, the more assistance can be given to the individual. Certainly measurement of important variables runs into problems of assessment, bias, and confounding. More "fine tuning" of instruments are required in order to address hese difficult problems. Many questions remain unanswered in regard to stress research in general and even more are created regarding injury as a stressful life event. Before a reliable and valid model can be formulated and tested he following research questions need to be considered; - What role does gender play, if any, in the adjustment of injured workers? Are here real differences between the sexes in this particular life stress or were the instruments not sensitive enough to detect hem? - Are social support and locus of control constructs too broad?h? Should they be fractionalized when used in stress researc - Are there other variables that might be significant such as income level, education, or age? - Since stress occurs over time, would Ion itudinal studies wih control groups demonstrate more significant resu ? Although there probably will never be he perfect design to examine work injury, at least an attempt has been made to increase he base of knowledge in his arena. APPENDICES APPENDIX A CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE FORM 81 .APPERHIEX.A ' Date CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE I. . understand that I have been ashed to participate in a study regarding disabled workers who have recently experienced a work-related injury. This will require ay answering a set of questionnaires. but ay naae will not be used on any of these questionnaires to protect my identity. At the present tine. I can honestly state I anrnot experiencing any lapses in aesory/aeaory problens that would interfere with ay recall.of dates/events. etc. However. I recognize that I say disniss ayself tron the study prior to coapleting the questionnaire it I so choose for any reason. I further understand that participation in this study will not produce any haraful psychological events to ay person. Additionally. I also understand that in no way is the continuation/teraination of ay disability benefits paid to ne connected to this study. Finally. I understand that any 82 responses I give will not be used by the researcher in a manner that would reveal ay identity or violate ay inherent right to privacy. Signed Witnessed by Date _ Date Code I Date Coapleted Interviewer APPENDIX B STRESSF UL LIFE EVENTS QUESTIONNAIRE 84 APPENDIX B Questionnaire. Porn 2 WHEEL madame]. 1. Single now and never married 4. Female 2. Divorced/Hidowed/Separated 5. hale 3. Harried/Living with Partner On 0 6. Number of dependent children not living in sane household with you at present . . (specifyI 7. Number of dependent children living in same household with you at present __________. Ispecifyl c o lo 1 c e a one 8. Factory Worker 15. Black 9. Office Worker 16. white (non managerialladninistrativel 10. ManagerialIAdministrative 17. Hispanic 11. Service (Haiter. waitress. 18. Other Hairdresser. etc.) (specify) 12. Skilled Trade 13. Business Owner 14. Other 0 0 B ne 19. American born 85 20. Foreign born Ispecify country) s. c o I To answer the questions below. mark an "X” in one or more of the columns to the right of each question. If the event in question has occurred to you within the past two years. indicate when it occurred by aarking the appropriate column: 0-6 aonths ago. 7-12 months ago. etc. It say be the case with some of the events below that you experienced then over sore than‘one of the tiae periods listed for the past two years. It so. mark all the appropriate columns. If the event has not occurred to you during the last two years for has'never occurred to you) mark the last coluan with an ’X'. Now go through the questionnaire and mark your recent life changes. A. HEALEB 9 Within the time 19-24 13-18 7-12 0-6 periods listed. mos. nos. mos. nos: Over 2 have you unnamed: m ass ass up mm 11:19.! 21. An illness or injury ' which: It) kept you in bed a week or more. or took you to the hospital? lb) was less serious than described above? 22. a aajor change in eating habits? 23. a sajor change in sleeping habits? 2‘. a change in your usual type and/or amount of recreation? 25. aajor dental work WORK 86 19-24 13-18 aos. aos. £32 532 7-12 Within the tine period listed. have you: 26. changed to a new type of work? 0-6 aos. Over 2 1391:3353st 27. 26. 29. 19-24 mos. ass changed your work hours or conditions? had a change in your responsibilities at work. such as: (check all that apply) IaI more ' responsibilities? ____ Ib) less I responsibilities? __ IcI promotion? Id) demotion? Ie) transfer? experienced troubles at work [check all that apply) la) with your boss? __ M with co-workers? IcI with persons under your supervision? 87 13-10 1-12 IO. . 0-6 mos. Over 2 ass Issues: lime 88 19-24 13—1e I08. I03. :32 . m ‘IdI other work troubles? 30. experienced a major business readjustment? ____ 31. retired? 32. experienced being: Ia) fired from work? Ibl laid off from work?___ 33. taken courses by sail or studied at home to . help you in your work? ____ HOME AND FAMILY Within the time periods listed. have you experienced: ' 34. a change in residence such as: Ia) a move within the same town or city?____ Ib) a move to a different town. city or state? 1-12 0-6 mos. Over 2 :22 mm lime 35. 36. 37. 36. 39. 89 19-24 13-16 7-12 mos. mos. nos. 132 £39 ESQ a change in family 'get-togethers'? a major change in the health or behavior or a family member' (illness. accidents. drug or disciplinary problems. etc.)? major change in your living conditions Ihome improveaents or a decline in your home or neighborhood)? the death of a spouse? 0-6 nos. Over 2 segmenting: the death of a: lcheck all that apply) In) lbl (c) a child? brother or sister?____ parents? 40. 61. NOTE: Id) 90 19-24 13—16 mos. mos. £39 232 other close faaily member? the death of a close friend? a change in the aarital status of your parents: lcheck all that apply) Ia) Ibl divorce? remarriage? (Questions 42-53 concern marriage. For persons never married. proceed to Item 5‘.) 42. marriage? 43. change in arguments with spouse? Ia) Ib) an increase in arguments with your spouse? ____ a decrease in arguments with your spouse? 7-12 I03 . 0-6 mos. Over 2 mMnAsstLer, 44. 45. 46. ‘7. ‘8. 91 19-24 13-16 7-12 0-6 mos.' mos. mos. mos. Over 2 as: no use mmmm; Is). no change in arguments. with spouse? in-law problems? a separation from spouse: IaI due to work? IbI due to marital problems? . a reconciliation with spouse? a divorce? a gain of a new family menber: IaI birth of a child? IbI adoption of a child? lo) a relative moving in with you? 49. SOs 51. 52. 53. 5N2 19-24 13-18 7-12 0-6 sos. £39 wife beginning or ceasing work outside the home? ____ wife beconing pregnant?____ a child leaving home: Ia) due to aarriage? IbI to attend college? Ic) for other reasons?____ wife have a miscarriage or abortion? . birth of a grandchild? ____ rsasounu awn SOCIAL . Within the time periods listed. have you experienced: 54. 55. a major personal achievement? - ____ a change in your personal habits Iyour dress. friends. life- style. etc.I? _ mos. mos. mos. Over 2 ass ass ass Tsar: Ass Essen 56. 57. 56. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 6S. sexual difficulties? beginning or ceasing school or college? change of school or college? a vacation? a change in your religious beliefs? 93 19-24 13-16 1-12 0-6 mos. mos. mos. mos. Over 2 ass-ass ass assmnessfisssr. a change in your social activities (clubs. movies. visiting)? a minor violation of the law? legal troubles resulting in your being in jail? a change in your political beliefs? a new. close personal relationship? so. 94 19524 13-16 7-12 0-6 mos. nos. mos. mos. Over 2 ass ass ass-sssl'smbssmg. an engagenent to marry? 67. a 'falling out' of a close personal relationship?‘ ____ 66. girlfriend (or boyfriend) problems? 69. a loss or damage of personal property? __.. 70. an accident? 71. a najor decision regarding your immediate future? FINANCIAL Within the time periods listed: have you: 72. 73. taken on a moderate purchase. such as a T.V.. car. freezer. etc.? taken on.a major purchase or a mortgage loan. such 'as a home. business. property. etc.? _ __.. _ __ 95 19-24 13-18 7-12 0-6 mos. mos. mos. mos. Over 2 assessessessIsaIsAssMsr 74. experienced a foreclosure on a mortgage or loan? 75. experienced a major change in finances: Icircle all that apply) Ia) increased income? ____ (b) decreased income? __ Ic) credit rating difficulties? 76. Number of years in present job lspecify) 77. Number of years in present company Ispecify) 1022!: Inot including income from disability benefits) (circle one) _ 76. 50-54.999 _ 82. 520.000-524.999 79. $5.000-59.999 83. 525,000-529,999 80. 510,000-514,999 04. $30.000-534.999 81. 515,000-519,999 85. $35,000 and above Code I Date Completed Interviewer APPENDIX C INTERNAL/EXTERNAL LOCUS OF CONTROL SCALE 97 IAPFEEHDIXICI Questionnaire. Form 3 Instrssilsnsi This is a questionnaire to find out the way in which certain important events in our society affect different people. Each iten consists of a pair of alternatives lettered a or b. Zlgasg nwmmmmmmnm WW ' Be sure to select the one you actually believe is to be more true rather than the one you think you should choose or the one you would like to be true. This is a measure of personal belief: obviously there are no right or wrong answers. In some instances you may discover that you believe both statements or neither one. In such cases, be sure to select the response you strongly believe to be more true as far as you're. concerned. Also try to respond to each item independently when making your choice: do not be influenced by your previous choices. 1. __a. Children get into trouble because their parents punish them too much. . __b. The trouble with most children nowadays is that their parents are too easy with them. 98 Hanonf the unhappy things in people's lives are partly due to bad luck. People's misfortunes result from the mistakes they make. . One of the major reasons why we have wars is because people don't take enough interest in politics. There will always be wars. no matter how hard people try to prevent them. In the long run people get the respect they deserve in this world. Unfortunately, an individual's worth often passes 'unrecognised no matter how hard he tries. The idea that teachers are unfair to students is nonsense. ‘ host students don't realise the extent to which their grades are influenced by accidental happenings. Without the right breaks one cannot be an effective leader. Capable people who fail to become leaders have not taken advantage of their opportunities. No matter how hard you try some people just don't like you. 10. 11. 12. 99 People who can't get others to like them don't understand how to get along with others. Heredity plays the major role in determining one's personality. It is one's experiences in life which determine what they're like. I have often found that what is going to happen will happen. Trusting to fate has never turned out as well for me as making a decision to take a definite course of action. In the case of the well prepared student there is rarely if ever such a thing as an unfair test. Many times exam questions tend to be so unrelated to course work that studying is really useless. Becoming a success is a matter of hard work. luck has little or nothing to do with it. Getting a good job depends mainly on being in the right place at the right time. The average citizen can have an influence in government decisions. This world is run by the few people in power. and there is not much the little guy can do about it. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. b. 100 When I make plans, I an almost certain that I can make them work. It is not always wise to plan too far ahead because many things turn out to be a matter or good or bad fortune anyhow. There are certain people who are just no good. There is some good in everybody. In my case getting what I want has little or nothing to do with luck. Nany times we might must as well decide what to do by flipping a coin. Who gets to be the boss depends on who was lucky enough to be in the right place first. Getting people to do the right thing depends on ability: luck has little or nothing to do with it. As far as world affairs are concerned, most of us are the victims of forces we can neither understand. nor' control. By taking an active part in political and social affairs the people can control world events. Host people don't realize the extent to which their lives are controlled by accidental happenings. There really is no such thing as 'luck'. 19. 2°. 21. 22. 23. 24. 101 One should always be willing to admit his mistakes. It is usually best to cover up one's mistakes. It is hard to know whether or not a person really likes you. how many friends.you have depends on how nice a person you are. In the long run the bad things that happen to use are balanced by the good ones. Nost misfortunes are the result of lack of ability, ignorance. laziness, or all three. With enough effort we can wipe out political corruption. It is difficult for people to have much control over the things politicians do in office. Sometimes I can't understand how teachers arrive at the grades they give. There is a direct connection between how hard I study and the grades I get. A good leader expects people to decide for themselves what they should do. A good leader makes it clear to everybody what their jobs are. 102 Nany times I feel that I have little influence over It is impossible for me to believe that chance or luck People are lonely because they don't try to be There's not much use in trying too hard to please people. if they like you. they like you. There is too much emphasis on athletics in high Team sports are an excellent way to build character. Sometimes.I feel that I don't have enough control over the direction my life is taking. host of the time I can't understand why politicians In the long run the people are responsible for bad government on a national as well as on a local level. 25. __a. the things that happen to me. __b. plays an important role in my life. 26. __a. friendly. __b. 27. __a. ' school. __b. 28. __.. What happens to me is my own doing. _;b. 29. __a. behave the way they do. __b. Code I Date Completed Interviewer APPENDIX D BECK DEPRESSION SCALE 104 PURPEHNDIXII) Questionnaire. Form 4 DIRECTIONS: On this questionnaire are groups of statements for you to read. After carefully reading each group of statements, pick out the pp; statement in each group which best describes the way you have been feeling during_thg_pg§3_ggnth. Circle the number which corresponds to the statement you picked on the numbered response line for each question. 1. 0 l 3. 0 1 2 I I do not feel sad. feel sad. am sad all the time and can‘t snap out of it. am so sad or unhappy that I can't stand it. am not particularly discouraged about the future. I feel discouraged about the future. I feel I have nothing to look forward to. I feel that the future is hopeless and that things cannot improve. I do not feel like a failure. I feel I have failed more than the average person. As I look back on my life. all I can see is a lot of failures. 3 I feel I am a complete failure as a person. NFOUNHOUNHOU U HHH HH 1CN5 get as much satisfaction out of things as I used to. don't enjoy things the way I used to. don't get real satisfaction out of anything anymore. an dissatisfied or bored with everything. don't feel particularly guilty. feel guilty a good part of the time. feel quite guilty most of the time. feel guilty all of the time. don't feel I am being punished. feel I may be punished. I expect to be punished. I I feel I am being punished. don't feel disappointed in myself. am disappointed in myself. an disgusted with myself. hate myself. don't feel I an any worse than anybody else. as critical of myself for my weaknesses or mistakes. blame myself all the time for my faults. blame myself for everything that happens. don't have any thoughts of killing nyself. have thoughts of killing myself, but I would not carry them out. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. UNHO U HOGAN 106 I would like to kill myself. I would kill myself if I had the chance. I don't cry anymore than usual. I cry more now than I used to. cry all the time now. used to be able to cry. but now I can't cry even though want to. am no more irritated now than I ever an. HD-{HHH get annoyed or irritated more easily than I used to. H feel irritated all the time now. I don't get irritated at all by the things that used to irritate me. I have not lost interest in other people. I am less interested in other people than I used to be. I have lost most of my interest in other people. I have lost all of my interest in other people. I make decisions about as well as I ever could. I put off making decisions more than I used to. I have greater difficulty in making decisions than before. I I can't make decisions at all anymore. I don't feel I look any worse than I used to. I am worried that I am looking old or unattractive. 15. 16. 17. 10. 107 I feel that there are permanent changes in my appearance that sake me look unattractive. I believe that I look ugly. I can work about as well as before. It takes an extra effort to get started at doing something. _ I have to push myself very hard to do anything. I can't do any work at all. I can sleep as well as usual. I don't sleep as well as I used to. I wake up 1-2 hours earlier than usual and find it hard to get back to sleep. I wake up several hours earlier than I used to and cannot get back to sleep. I don't get more tired than usual. I get tired more easily than I used to. I get tired from doing almost anything. I am too tired to do anything. My appetite is no worse than usual. fly appetite is not as good as it used to be. My appetite is much worse now. I have no appetite at all anymore. l9. 0 20. 0 21. 0 N 108 I haven't lost much weight, if any. lately. I have lost more than 5 pounds. I have lost more than 10 pounds. I have lost more than 15 pounds. I am no more worried about my health than usual. I am worried about physical problems such as aches and pains: or upset stomach; or constipation. I am very worried about physical problems and it's hard to think of much else. I am so worried about my physical problems. that I cannot think about anything else. I have not noticed any recent change in my interest in sex. I an less interested in sex than I used to be. I am much less interested in sex now. I have lost interest in sex completely. Code I Date Completed Interviewer APPENDIX E SOCIAL SUPPORT DIMENSION SCALE 110 .APPEQUILX E Questionnaire, Form 5 Instructions: We would like to learn more about your interactions with (pg; groups of people: I1) Close friends. (2) co-workers, (3) your family. and I4) professionals (doctors. teachers. therapists. religious leaders. etc.). Please be careful to use the appropriate response scale for each question. Use only one response per question. 0 1 2 3 hoderate . Very or Great Nsns Lillie Azszsss 0:31 In general, how much EHQIIQNAL_§QEEQBI do you receive from: la. Friends? 1b. Co-workers? lc. Your family? 1d. Helping professionals? (Social Workers. Counselors. Psychologists. Physicians. Psychiatrists. Ministers. etc.) 111 0 l 2 3 Hoderate Very or ‘Great fish: L131: 81339: 12:31 In general. how much EBA;TI§AL_A§§1§TAN§£ (money. food. housing. services, etc.) do you receive from: 2a. Friends? _ _ __ __.. 2b. Co-workers? __ __.._ _ _— 2c. Your family? __ __ __ _ 2d. Helping professionals? (Social Workers . Counselors. Psychologists. Physicians. Psychiatrists. Hinisters. etc.) _. __ __ In general. how much W do you receive from: 3a. Friends? __ __ __ 3b. Co-workers? . __.. __ __._' _— 3c. Your family? 112 O 1 2 3 Hoderate Very or Great Ilsnshiitlsessrsssm 3d. Helping professionals? (Social Workers. Counselors. Psychologists, Physicians, Psychiatrists. Hinisters. etc.) In general. how much AQ!1§£_Q£_INIQBHAIIQN do you receive from: 4a. Friends? 4b. Co-workers? 4c. Your family? 4d. Helping professionals? (Social Workers. Counselors. Psychologists. Physicians. Psychiatrists. Hinisters, etc.) 113 In general. how satisfied have you been with the amount of SflQILQflAL_SQEEQBI you have received from: O 1 2 3 Completely Somewhat Somewhat Completely , unsatisfied flsssiisfisd Ssiisfisd Satisfied 5a. Friends? 5b. Co-workers? 5c. Your family? 5d. Helping ’ professionals? (Social Workers, Counselors. Psychologists. Physicians. Psychiatrists. Hinisters. etc.) In general. how satisfied have you been with amount of {BASILQAL ASSISTANQE (money, food, housing. services. etc.) you have received from: 114 O 1 2 3 Completely Somewhat Somewhat Completely Dhssiisfisd stsiisfisd Satisfied Satisfied 6a. Friends? 6b. Co-workers? 6c. Your family? 6d. Helping professionals? (Social Workers. Counselors. ' Psychologists. Physicians, Psychiatrists. Ministers. etc.) In general. how satisfied have you been with the amount of W you I... received from: 7a. Friends? 7b. Co-workers? 115 O 1 2 3 Completely Somewhat Somewhat Completely mem 8c. Your family? 8d. Helping professionals? (Social Wbrkers. Counselors. Psychologists. Physicians. Psychiatrists. Ministers, etc.) In general. how satisfied have you been with the amount of W you have received tron: 9a. Friends? 9b. Co-workers? 9c. Your family? 116 0 1 I 2 3 Completely Somewhat Somewhat Completely Unsatisfied Ilssstisfisd Satisfied Satisfied 9d. Helping professionals? (Social Workers, Counselors. Psychologists. Physicians. Psychiatrists, Ministers. etc.) Code I Date Interviewer APPENDIX F COPING WITH DISABILITY INVENTORY 118 APPENDIX I" Questionnaire, Form 6 Rate each of the following statements in one of the columns on the right side of the page that best describes your preference or leanings. Answer according to your present situation (current feelings). I 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often! Almost 3:131! 52192! 31193 22:93:81 812321 1. I obtain intonation about my body in relation to my disability. 2. I am involved in social. political and/or non-work activities. 3. I am aware of my personal needs and 6011601118 e 4. 1 153 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Sone- Often] Almost Basel: Sslsss times Fragment diners As a result of my 'disability. I tend to view life as. having both meaning and purpose. I think about my disability. I find different things to do during my free time. I am able to express my anger. I can tolerate anger directed toward me. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 120 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often] Almost BarslrSsldseiinssEmsniAlm I feel like a victim of fate or misfortune because of my disability. I have close love relationships. I experience emotional $22888 . I hold on to my opinions even though others may not agree. I consider my disability an inconvenience. I feel that I have to be on my guard in interaction with others. I help and encourage others. 16. 11. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 121 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often/ Almost Rarslrfisldsmiimssmsusnihlmn I use fantasy and imagination to develop options and opportunities in my life. I an optimistic and hopeful about my life. I participate in social organizations. 'I an involved in removing disability barriers and prejudice. I enjoy life. I an able to handle frustrating experiences. I am able to obtain material comforts. I have a positive opinion about myself. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. ' my body. 122 1 2 3 Never/ ' Some- Bannisldsatimes I accept that my body looks and functions differently from others. 4 5 Often/ Almost Erssusnidlsszs I desire relationships that include intimacy and trust. I pay close attention to I cannot stand ambiguity or uncertainty. I can point to real achievements in my life. I think of my disabilities as the worst thing that has happened to me. I see myself as no longer disabled in my day dreams. I think that my disability has advantages. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 123 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Sone- Often/ Almost BanisSsisssiiaestresusniAlnn I feel comfortable with looking at myself in the mirror. I care for the people and things in my life. I am aware of the difference between loving someone and needing someone's love. I as comfortable when others do not accept my beliefs. I am satisfied with myself even though I may be unemployed. I live in the 'here and now' rather than in the past. 3°. 39. 40. 41. ‘2. ‘3. 44. 124 1 2 3 4 5 Never] - Some- Often] Almost Barslzfieldsmiimsatmsnidlnn I can accept compliments and recognition from other people. I think my life is challenging and exciting. I perceive problems as opportunities for growth. I am responsible for making other people happy. I like myself and accept my failings. I seek and obtain specific information to solve problems. I base my decisions on my future goals. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. SOs 51. 52. 125- 1 2 3 4 5 never] Some- Often] Almost mmnmzmmm I feel comfortable about asking others for support and assistance. I have problems in communicating with others. I as willing to take calculated risks. I initiate interactions with others. I see opportunities in sy life as limited. I use professional assistance when needed. I reflect before and after my actions. I sake efforts to overcome and solve my problens. S3. 5‘. 55. 56. 57. $8. 59. 126 - 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often] Alnost mmnmmmmamn I am positively influenced by persons apart from my family. I can laugh at myself and with others about life happenings that are connected with my disability. I am cautious in my behavior. I seek advice from other disabled persons. I find myself cosplying to the expectations of others. I back away from difficult situations. I like receiving compliments and recognition from other people. 60. 61. 62. 63. 6‘. 65. 66. 127 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often] Alnost mummmmnmnm I understand the nonverbal messages of others toward me. I examine alternative solutions to problems. I feel helpless in dealing with my disability. I use self-control in expressing my feelings. I attribute my disability to fate. I display my enotional reactions to stressful situations. I try to influence the direction of events 'toward personally determined goals. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 128 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often] Almost 83:211. firings Ills: Ercgusnt 51!!!! I mentally rehearse . responses to events that will or night happen. I consider myself to be the source of control over events in ay life. I experiment with different ways of dealing with disability-related problems. I evaluate sy behavior by my own internal standards. I try to focus on other areas of sy life that are more rewarding when I am troubled by my life. I experience sadness. I experience fear. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 19. 129 1 2 3 4 5 Never] Some- Often] Almost mmmmm I an alert to changes in my body that may affect my health. I give myself presents. treats or nurture myself in other ways. I take responsibility for a problem rather than blaming myself for it. I experience grief in relation to my disability. I look forward to the future as an opportunity for further growth. I perceive problems as opportunities for growth. Code I Date Completed Interviewer APPENDIX G LOGBOOK 131 .APPEIHILX‘G upcoesou conmaeoeanam acsom you uueaeaasa xoonmod Hon oeoz APPENDIX H INTRODUCTORY PARTICIPATION STATEMENT 133 APPENDIX H INTRODUCTORY PARTICIPATION STATEMENT My colleagues and I are conducting research regarding industrial injury and stress. The purpose of this study is to discover how injured workers deal with this life event. We would appreciate it very much if you would answer these questions as honestly and completely as possible. Be assured that all answers are STRICTLY CONFIDENTIAL. Your personal answers will not be divulged under any circumstances. The questionnaires will be identified by code numbers and not by name so that no connection can be made between any individual and their answers. Interest is in the answers of all respondents taken as a group rather than the answers of any one respondent. All results will be reported in general statistical form and no reference will be made to any individual. I anticipate that there will be no risk in to you in filling out this questionnaire packet, nor will there be any benefits to you. Your participation or non-participation has no bearing on your rehabilitation interview. Thank you for your cooperation in this matter. If there should be any questions at a later date, please feel free to call me at 559-1406. APPENDIX I UCRIHS APPROVAL LETTER 135 .APPEDHDIX I MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY usmsmr COIHITIEE or»: result" mvou'tsc usr unsure . mcmom . «nu-toes wuum sonnets (terms. 2» ADMISISTIATION surmise ism m-zm July 22, 1987 Ms. Elaine H. Tripi 505 Victoria Square Brighton, Michigan 68116 Dear Ms. Tripi: Subject: Proposal Entitled, "The Industrially Injured Worker: Locus of Control Orientation, Gender and Social Support as Moderators of Stressful Life Events and Adaptation to Disability" UCRIHS' review of the above referenced project has now been completed. I am pleased to advise that since the reviewers‘ comments have been satisfactorily addressed, the conditional approval given by the Committee at its July 6, 1987 meeting has now been changed to full approval. You are reminded that UCRIHS approval’is valid for one calendar year. If you plan to continue this project beyond one year, please make provisions for obtaining appropriate UCRIHS approval prior to July 6, 1988. Any changes in procedures involving human subjects must be reviewed by the UCRIHS prior to initiation of the change. UCRIHS must also be notified promptly of any problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects during the course of the work. Thank you for bringing this project to our attention. If we can be of any future help, please do not hesitate to let us know. Sincerely, I +zg..ech- Henry E. Bredeck, h.D. Chairman, UCRIHS jms cc: Dr. William Hinds ”fl -' u as Allen-us" A. n»- "fly-ml Opp-ensue laurel-lane LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES AI-lssa. I. (Ed.). (1982). n r n I h h I . New York: Academic Press. Anderson, C. R. (1977). Locus of control, coping behaviors, and performance in a stress setting. I li P I , 5g 446-451. Antonnucci, T. & Depner, C. (1982). Social support and informal helping relationships InT Wills (Ed..) WWW». (pp. 141- 160). New York: Academic Press. Bamet, R. C., Biener, L., & Baruch. (Eds). (1987). W. New York: The Free Press. Beck, A. T. (1967). i 'ni l i I New York: Harper rand Row. Beck, A. T. (1972). Measuring depression: The de ression inventory. In T. A. Williams, M. M. Katz, 8. J. A. Shields (Eds, W i l iv iI (pp. 229-302). Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office. Beck, A..T, Rush, A..J Shaw, B..F, &Emery, J. (1979). mm gem New York: Guilford Press. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. &Jarbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric propertiesofthe Beck Depression Inventory?100 Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical W5w Beck, A..T Ward. C..H, Mendelson, M., Mark. J. &Erbaugh J. (1961). An are??? for measuring depression W. 1. Behan, R. C. 8. Hirshfield, A. H. (1963). The accident process: Toward amore rational treatment of industrial injuries. WM Association155. 300-306 Behan, R. C. & Hirshfield, A. H. (1966). Disability without disease or accident. WM 12. 655-661 Belle, D. (1987). Gender differences in the social moderators of stress. In R. C. Barnett, L. Biener, 8. G. K. Baruch. (Eds). W (pp. 257-279). New York: The Free Press. Benassi, V. A. Sweeney, Paul D, &Sufour, C. L. (1988). Is there a relationship between LOC orientation and depression? W 13.519.661qu 91. 357-367 Berkowitz, M. (1985). W. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University. Bureau of Economic Research. 136 137 Billings, A. G. 8. Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of the coping responses and social resources in attentuating the stress of life events. MM W. .4. 139-157 Bloom, B. L. 8.Caldwell, R. A. (1981). Sex differences In adjustment during the process of marital separation WM 4.5. 693-701. Bloom, B. L. Hodges, W. F. 8.Caldwell, R. A. (1982). Apreventive program for the newly separated: Initial evaluation. W W 1.0. 251-264 Caldwell, M. A., 8. Peplau, L. A. (1982). Sex differences in same-sex friendship. 5.6830155. 8. 721-732. Caldwell, R. A., 8.Bloom, B. L. (1982). Social support: Its structure and impact 327mg? disruption W. 15. Caldwell, R. A., Pearson, J. L., 8. Chin, R. J. (1984). The stress moderating effects of social support in the context of neg-e:7 and locus of control. i Caldwell, 8. Rinehart, M. A. (1988). Relationship of personality to individual differences in the use of pa and source of social supports. Caplan. G. (1974). WWW New York: Basic Books. Cassel, J. (1974). Psychosocial processes and stress": theoretical formulation. WW. 4. 471-482 Cohen, L. H., McGovern, J., Fooskas, S., 8. Rose, S. (1984). Positive life events and social support and the relationship between life stress and psoyhologioal disorder W. 12. 567-87 Cohen, S., 8. Edwards, J. R. ( 1986) Personality characteristics as moderators of the relationship between stress and disorder. In R. W. J. Neufeld (Ed. ), (pp 201-240) W. New York: May. Cohen, S., 8. WIlls, T. A. (1985). Stress, support and the buffering hypothesis. W. ".28 310-357 Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. W119 Medicine. 55. 300-814. Crotty, P., 8. Kulys. R. (1985). Social support networks: The views of gooqizaoghrenic clients and their significant others. $993M, 3.. 138 Dean, A., 8. Ensel, W. M. (1982). Modelling social support, life events. competence and depression in the context of age and sex. ,19, 392-408. Dean, A., 8. Lin, N. (1977). The stress buffering role of social support. Mme! W. 155. 403-417 De Longis, A., Coyne, J. C., Dakof, G. Folkman, S. 8.Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Relationship of daily hassles, uplifts and major life events to health status. Healtnfisxchdlddi 1.119-136. Dohrenwend, B. S.. 8 Dohrenwend, B. P. (Eds). (1981). W W. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Ell, Kathleen O. (1986). Coping with serious illness: On integrating research, assessments and intervention WWW Medicine 1.6. 335-56. Etzion, D. (1984). Moderatin effect of social support on the stress - bumout relationship leumaLLppledflxcboledy. 55. 615-622 Fox. D. J. (1969). W New York: Holt. Rinehart and Winston. Gentry, W. D., 8. Kobasa, S. C. (1984). Social and psychological resources mediating stress-illness relationships in humans. In W. D. Gentry. (Ed. ), 53W (pp 87-116.) New York: Guilford Press. Gilligan. C. (1982). WWW 99191991119111 Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. Gore, 8. L. (1981). Stress-buffering functions of social supports: An raisal and clarification of research models. In B. S. Dohrenwend B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds), Ilif v n ir (pp. 202-222). New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. Gottleib, B. H. (1985). Assessing and stren hening the impact of social supporton mental health. W 9, 93-300. Gottleib. B. H. (Ed.). (1981). WWW. BeveIIy Hills, California: Sage Publications. Habif, V. L., 8. Lahey, B. B. (1980). Assessment of the life stress-depression relationship: The use of social support as a moderator variable. 999mg! 9159mm. 2. 168-173. Hamlin. M. (1982). WWW 991m. Unpublished dissertation, University of Detroit. Hirsch, B. J. (1981). Networks and the ceping process: Creating personal oornmunities InB H Gotflieb(Ed)§ecI_a_nemoLk_s_am_sccmLaIppect (pp. 149-170). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 139 Hirschfield, A. H., 8. Behan, R. .(1963). The accident process: Etiological considerations of industrial injuries. WWW Association.15.6.193-199 Holmes, T. H. 8. Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment scale. MM Wm 22, 205-208. House, J. S. (1985). Barriers to work stress: I. SocIaI support InW D. Gentry, H. Benson and C. J. deWoIff (Eds) r Im ansihealtn (pp. 157-180). Martinus Nijhoff. The Netherlands. Huisaini, B. Neff, J. Newbrook, J. R., 8 Moore, M. C. (1982). The stress-buffering role of sociaI support and personal competence among the rural married. JeumaLdmdmmummmcmdgy. 1.0. 409-426 Huisaini, B. A... 8 von Frank, A. (1985). Life events. coping resources, and depression: A longitudinal study of direct, buffering, and reciprocal effects. BMW. 5. 11-136 Jenkins, R. (1985). New horizons: Sex differences in minor psychiatric morbidity. EsxcuesemafieMedicine Monograph Supplement 7 Johnson, J.H ,8 Sarason, l. G. (1978). Life stress, depression and anxiety: Internal-external controlzzaszog- 2rc')r18<>derator variable. 199mm Kahn, R. L., 8 Antonucci, T. C. (1980). Convo s over the life course: Attachment, roles and social support. W m4. 253-286. Kessler, R. C. (1982). A disaggregation of the relationshi between socioeconomic status and psychological distress. mm erd. 12. 752-64. Kessler, R. C., 8 McLeod, J. S. (1982). Social support and mental health in community samples. In S. Cohen 8 S. L. Syme (Eds.,) W health (pp. 219-240). New York: Academic Press. Kessler, R. C., Price, H., 8 Wortman, C. B(1985). Social factors in psychopathology: Stress, social support, and coping processes. Annual W 55. 531 -72 Knight, R. G. (1984). Some general population norms for the short form Beck Depression Inventory, m I lini l P l ,_4_9. 751-753. Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and health: An inquiry into hardiness. W. 51. 1- 11 Krause, N.(1985) Stress, control beliefs and psychological distress: The problem of response bias W 11.11-20 Krause, Neal (1987). Understanding the stress process: Linkin social support with locus of control beliefs. MW, 4_7_, 5 9-93. 140 Krause, N. 8 Stryker, S. (1984). Stress and well- -being: The bufferi role of locus of control beliefs. W 15. 205-20. Kulchami, M. (1985). nivli' fninrmn ini m'r in hvir II r n i l i, Unpublished dissertation, Michigan State University. Kulchami, M., 8Bl0m, G. (1985). MW. Unpublished manuscript. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, University Center for lntemational Rehabilitation. Kundu. M M. (1983). WWW WW of control. wo motivation and wgrk hisjgnry varianlaaz nagn Igrajgry singly. Unpublished Dissertation. Michigan State University. La Rocco, J. M., House, J. S., 8 French, R. P., Jr. (1980). Social support. ational stress. and health W. 2.1! 202'218. Lazarus, R. 8., Cohen, J. B., Folkman, S. Kanner, A. 8 Schafer, C. (1980). Psychological stress and adaptation: Some unresolved issues. In H. Selye (Ed..) WW. (pp 90-117) New York: Van Nost and Reinhold Company. Lazarus, R. S. 8Folkman, S. (1985). W. New York: Springer. Leavy,R ..L (1983). Social support and compliance: A selective review and critique of treatment integ and outcome measurement. W BELMQQELQ 11. 1329-13 8. Lefcourt, H. M. (1966). Internal versus external locus of control of reinforcement: A review. I i I II ' .59, 206-220. Lefcourt, H. M. (1981). Locus of Control and stressful life events. In B. S. Dohrenwend 8 B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds.,) WWW gantaxts (pp. 157—166). New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University ress Lefcourt, H. M. (1982). W (2nd ed.). New York: Academic Press. Lefcourt. H. M. (1984). Locus of control and stressful life events. In B. S. Dohrenwend 8 Bruce P. Dohrenwend (Eds), f l Iif v n n W (pp. 157-166). New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. Levenson, H., Hirshfield, M. L., Hirshfield, A., 8 Dzubay, B. (1983). Recent life events and accidents: The role of sex differences. WWI] $1655. 9. 4.11, 141 Lin, N., Simeone, R. S., Ensel, W. M., and Kuo, W. (1979). Social support, stressful life events, and illness: A model and an emperical test. 19111051 WWI/1121.11. 2Q. 108-19- Mechanic, D. (1976). Sex, illness, illness behavior and the use of health services. Jeumalettlumanfitteee. 2.2-6. Miller, P. M. 8 lngham-L1415 1(35(1976). Friends, confldants and symptoms. 55531155151661. - Miller, S. M., 8 Kirsch. N. (1987). Sex differences in cognitive coping with stress. In R. C. Barnett, L. Biener, 8. G. K. Baruch (Eds) Gandanand 555.55. (pp. 278-307). New York: The Free Press. Mitzel. H. E. (Ed). (1982). Encyclopediaeieducaflmalmeard: (Vol. 111). New York: Free Press. Moreau, FA (1983). An investigation of locus of control and achievement motivation in physically handicapped community college students. Dissertadenflzsnaciemdmaficnal. 44. ((3-a) 712). Mullen, B. 8 Suls, J. (1982). ‘Know thyself": Stressful life changes and the ameliorative effect of private self-consciosness.,lgurna1_gf_Eam_m_e_n1al W 18.. 43-55 Myersand J., Lindenthal, J., 8 Pepper, M. P. (1975). Life events, social integration andgsyalggtric sanptornotology..LeumaLdf_BealtI1_and_$.eciaI_Behaxier. Nielson, A. C. 8. William, T. A. (1980). Depression in ambulatory medical patients Prevalence by self-report uestionnaire iand recognition by nonpsychiatric physicians. r ,3_7_. 7999-1004. Nunnally, J. C. (1978). W. New York: McGraw-Hill. Overton, S. (1987). i i’ n l i i n i ' i ' I wi h ’ ° . Unpublished dissertation, Michigan State University. Farm i.L (Ed). (1983). Wen. ,.V0Iumel Geneva: International Labour Office. Paykel, E. 8., Emms, E. M., Fletcher, J., 8 Rassaby, F. S. (1980). Life events and social su port in puerperal depression. W Payne. R. L., 8 Jones, G. J. (1987). Measurement and methodological Issues in social support. In S.V .Kasl 8 CL Cooper (Eds) §t_ns§_and_neann S ' (pp. 167-205). Chichester: John Wiley 8 ons. Pearlin. L. I., 8Schooler, 01.413731). The structureofcoping. WM addfidcialjenaxidr. - 142 Peterson, 0., 8. Seligman, M. E. P. (1983). Learned helplessness and victimization. mm 2, 103-116. Piotrowski, C., Sherry, D., 8 Keller, J. W. (1985). Psychodiagnostic test usage: A survey of the Society for Personality Assessment. Mam W49. 115-119. Plumb, M. M. 8 Holland, J. (1977). Comparative studies of psychological function in patients wrth advanced cancer, I: Self-reported depressive symptoms BsxcbesdmafieMmicine. 55. 264-279 Rahe, R. H. (1975). Epidemological studies of life change and illness. W6133-141. Rosenbaum, M., 8 Raz, D. (1977). Denial, locus of control and depression among physicall disabled and non-disabled men. Jgnmal gt QIInIQaI Esxebgidgx. 55. 72-676. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external locus of reinforcement. h l IM n 99, (Whole #609). Rotter, J. B. (1975). Some problems and misconceptions related to the construct of internal vs external control reinforcement. igmnang WM. 45 56-67. Sandler, I. N..(1980) Social support, resources, stress and maladjustment of poorchildren WW 5. 41 -.52 Sandler, I. N., 8 Lakey, B. (1982). Locus of control as a stress moderator: The role of control perceptions and social support. Amarigan Jgnrnal gf CommunihLEamgldgx. 10. 65—80. Schwab, J. J., Bialow, M., Brown, J. M., 8. Holzer, C. E. (1967)M Diagnosing depression in medical inpatients. Annals gt Intarnal Madigina, 61, 695-707. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). W. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Selye, H. (1956). 1015115811116. NewYork: McGraw Hill. Selye, H. (1973). The evolution of the stress concept. W, 91, 691-699. Shrout, Patrick E. (1984). Scalin of stressful life events. In B. S. Dohrenwend 8. Bruce P. DohrenwendI ds..) WWW: (pp. 29-47). New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. Srull, T. K., 8 Karabenick, S. A. (1975). Effects of personality-situation locus of control congruence W. 5.2.. 61 7-628. 143 Steer, R. A., Beck, A. T., 8 Garrison. B. (1986). Applications of the Beck Depression Inventory. In N. Sartorius 8T. A. Ban (Eds) Aaaasarnantel gl_a9_r_a§_ai_ee (pp. 121-142). Geneva. Switzerland: World Heath Organization. Stroebe, M. S., 8 Stroebe, W. (1983). Who suffers more? Sex differences in health risksofthe widowed. I i IB II i 99, 279-301. Tesch. S. A. (1983). Review of friendship development across the life span. Humanfleueidmcm. 25. 266-276. Thoits, P. A. (1982). Conceptual. methodological, and theoretical problems in studying social support as a buffer against life stress. Jeemalelfialaln W. 25. 145-159 Toves, C.. Schill, T., 8 Ramanaiah. N. (1981). Sex differences, intemaI-extemal apps of control and vulnerability to life stress. P l i l R . 49, Tuck. M. (1983). Psychological and sociological aspects of industrial injury. JemLoiBebabil'iatien. 5. 20-25 Verbrugge, L. M. (1985). Gender and health: An update on h otheses and evidence. rn I H I ' l h vi r,29, 156-1 2. Wallerstein, J., 8. Kelly. J. (1980). Sgrviying the breakup. New York: Basic Books. Weinstein, M. R. (1978). The concept of the disability process. WJgM-W. Weiss, L., 8 Lowenthal,M .(1975). Life-course perspectives on friendship. In M. L. Lowenthal, M. Thumder, and D. Chunboga (Eds) Eeleagaseflila (163-185). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Wheaton, B. (1985). Models for the stress-bufferin functions of coping resources. n l f i IB h vi r, ,352-364. Wickersham, J. F. (1983). J[Ed‘sability management: Key to cost savings at 3M. WELL Wills T. A., 8Lan%ner. T. S. (1980). Socioeconomic status and stress. an. L. Kutash & l- Schlesinger (Eds..) Wander (pp 159-173). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.