.9; if! I Ifhvlg‘. f ‘d . v-Uflval’..u {8%, \f- O to; 1.! .... 4.? Itcoflntutd .a. lifelvrbu‘v a: .3 .13), u”...- . . .01.”... I. rtvnir 3.311%: 13.1 a 1. sh»? I .5 3L .1 .r 3,4. 1"! i j ' are}? no u. .n o . .flo} : com”! 5"”...- . g 06 I I: It. vhf...) two... 0 "4" "III I... o.- ..lur! .arli. 1..., . at!» :5 , 5A!- I: f ltflo'9'“.. u.) v Ir . ,fo 9.... : ;fc). {.5405} r‘ Illfllfiiuv. J a I inHXI—p: Nu... tacit )- tllévfluva ‘ If?!) g a... $3 5.4.4.3 . ”1. on, I! I a: t. . 1?! o 1.13 . lbw u figuonoJII-n . t ‘v ‘ Alaisfia r. ”Wag“? v 2. 4.35947 :01: 00 35341”... if I II VI") Ill.- flamm. . :4» VII .11 . I I). all... , .- 01.34. f 3.. I. :5 l! 1,2!) 1.”: . ff .3. I I .13 o l‘ l .5} . «.8... r)! .0. ’l;‘ {K'nvffrlu’ a A, on." . ’LINH‘. If! ur pry I If!» V (I; ITY LIBRARI Illil’lilillili‘ilillMilli 3 1293 00791 4553 LIBRARY latchlgan Shh l University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled The Influence of Acquiring the Family Eldercare Role on Female Employment Adaptation presented by Susan Tesch Franklin has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein Family and Child Ecology Ww ,0. 4414/ Major professor Date 11/4/92 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity lnditution own”: THE INFLUENCE OF ACQUIRING THE FAMILY ELDERCARE ROLE ON FEMALE EMPLOYMENT ADAPTATION By Susan Tesch Franklin A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 1992 Del interaction Pm’ious c “Mined 1 0“ lhe emfi impact of ml'olveme: UKUGQ comallOng ilCNemar p Van: immediate ll women. WW rammed s; ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF ACQUIRING THE FAMILY ELDERCARE ROLE ON FEMALE EMPLOYMENT ADAPTATION By Susan Tesch Franklin Demographic and social trends precipitate concern about work and family interactions, but eldercare’s influence on employment is relatively unknown. Extending previous cross-sectional research about family care’s effect on employment, this study examined the influence of acquiring the eldercare role for a physically disabled relative on the employment adaptation of women at role inception and three months later. The impact of caregiver/care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement on three employment adaptation types, Type I (maintain, but alter work), Type II (leave of absence), and Type III (quit/ retire) was investigated. Analyses included correlations, oneway ANOVA, paired and independent t—tests, contingency tables, McNemar procedures, multiple and logistic regressions, and discriminant analysis. Varied adaptations occurred at both time periods. A major finding is the immediate influence of acquiring the eldercare role on the employment adaptation of women. Women reporting full-time or part-time employment prior to the caregiver role recounted substantial changes in that status at Time 1; they did not wait until Time 2. Caregivers reported using less employment adaptive behaviors at Time 2 than at Time 1. Type III probabilit Fl lht caregi' l6) includ fessionals, the mm ism168, an] Diverse variables influenced adaptation at Time 1. Caregiver/ care recipient characteristics were more influential with Type I and Type II adaptations while family support and caregiver involvement variables were better predictors of Type III adaptation. At Time 2, no variables anticipated Type I or Type II adaptations, but household income and hours of supervision offered good explanatory power for Time 2, Type III adaptation. The use of Time 1, Type I adaptation did not influence the probability of Time 2, Type II or Type III adaptations. Findings indicate the need to: (a) recognize the immediate influence of assuming the caregiver role, (b) increase workplace awareness of potential work/family conflicts, (c) include work/family care content in the educational preparation of family pro- fessionals, ((1) provide socio-emotional services for families, (e) help policy makers alter the traditional family image, (f) champion national policies supportive of work/family issues, and (g) consider ethnicity and relationship quality in future family/work research. Copl'righ SUSAN '. 1992 Capyright by SUSAN 'I'ESCH FRANKLIN 1992 Dedicated in loving memory of my father Carl E. Tesch March 27, 1917 August 27, I992 guidance availabili my disse Rosema: Their su data set I W'Orke SOfIWaI made [l Sch001 Stepha] ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Thanks to Dr. Barbara Ames, my advisor and committee chairperson, for the guidance and support she has provided throughout my graduate program. Her availability, encouragement (through good times and bad), and insightful comments about my dissertation made a difficult task easier. Thanks to the members of my graduate committee (Dr. Sharon King, Dr. Rosemary Walker, and Dr. June Youatt) for their interest and support of my progress. Their suggestions regarding my dissertation gave my work added dimension. Thanks to Dr. Charles Given and to Dr. Barbara Given for granting access to the data set I used. The Givens also provided my first exposure to research during the time I worked with them as a graduate research assistant. Special thanks to my dear husband, Greg. He not only gave me strong encouragement and support, but also kept my computer going, installed the needed software, learned to be a great cook, and ignored the dust! His quiet confidence in me made the stressful years of graduate school manageable. Special thanks also go to my daughter, Stephanie. Most mothers returning to school do not have a college senior willing to share an apartment, provide campus orientation, study together, and think the whole process is fun! My campus year with Stephanie will always be a special memory. vi vii Thanks to my son, Jeff, who teased me about all the computer time, but also gave my tired neck a quick rub and told me to drive safely on my many trips to East Lansing. Thanks to my mother, Evelyn Tesch, and my mother—in-law, Emalee Franklin; they (and my father) never doubted my ability to accomplish this goal. Also, the support of my brother, sisters, and their families bolstered me when I needed it most. This research was supported by grant ”Caregiver Responses to Managing Elderly Patients at Home” (NIA #2 R01 A0065 84), C. W. Given, Ph.D., Principal Investiga- tOI'. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ...................................... xi LIST OF FIGURES ...................................... xiv CHAPTER I: Introduction ................................... 1 Scope of the Problem ................................... 2 Statement of the Problem ................................. 8 Purpose of the Study .................................... 9 Relationship of this Study to Grant #2 R01 AGO6584 .............. 10 Significance and Generalizability ........................... 11 Conceptual Framework ................................. 13 Human Ecology Theory .............................. l3 Choice and Exchange Theory ........................... 15 Assumptions ..................................... 15 Assumptions of the Ecological Perspective ................ 15 Assumptions of Choice and Exchange Theory .............. l7 Assumptions of this Study .......................... 17 Limitations ......................................... 18 CHAPTER II: Review of Literature ............................ 20 Influence on Employees and Families ........................ 20 Influence on the Employer ............................... 25 Influence on Society ................................... 28 Conceptual Framework ................................. 29 Human Ecological Approach ........................... 29 Choice and Exchange Theory ........................... 34 Summary .......................................... 36 CHAPTER III: Methods ................................... 37 Research Objectives ................................... 41 Research Design ..................................... 41 Research Questions and Hypotheses ......................... 43 Research Variables .................................... 48 Dependent Variables ................................ 49 Type I Employment Adaptation (maintain, but alter work) ....... 49 Arrive Late/ Leave Early ......................... 49 viii ix Miss Work without Pay ......................... 49 Take Sick/Personal Days ........................ 49 Change Work Hours ........................... 50 Refuse I ob or Promotion ........................ 50 Kept from Job Hunt/Better Job ..................... 50 Other Work Effects ............................ 51 Type 11 Employment Adaptation (leave of absence) ........... 52 Type 111 Employment Adaptation (quit/retire) .............. 52 Independent Variables ............................... 52 Caregiver Relationship to Care Recipient ................. 52 Caregiver Age ................................. 53 Caregiver Marital Status ........................... 53 Caregiver Education .............................. 5 3 Caregiver Occupation ............................. 54 Caregiver Household Income ........................ 54 Employment Status (prior to caregiving) .................. 55 Current Employment Status ......................... 55 Co-Residence with Care Recipient ..................... 56 Care Recipient Age .............................. 56 Care Recipient Gender ............................ 56 Family Network ................................ 57 Family Help (amount) ............................. 57 Family Assistance (frequency) ........................ 57 Caregiver Total Involvement ......................... 58 Hours of Physical Care ............................ 60 Hours of Supervision ............................. 60 Instrumentation ...................................... 60 Sampling Procedure ................................... 66 Data Collection Techniques ............................... 70 Analysis of Data ..................................... 71 CHAPTER IV: Results .................................... 93 Research Question 1 ................................... 94 Research Question 2 ................................... 99 Research Question 3 .................................. 101 Research Question 4 .................................. 110 Research Question 5 .................................. 118 Research Question 6 .................................. 118 Research Question 7 .................................. 121 Research Question 8 .................................. 126 Research Question 9 .................................. 128 Research Question 10 ................................. 128 Research Question 11 ................................. 132 Research Question 12 ................................. 137 X CHAPTER V: Discussion ................................. 149 Summary of Findings ................................. 150 Time 1 ........................................ 152 Type I (maintain, but alter work) Employment Adaptation ...... 152 Type II (leave of absence) Employment Adaptation .......... 153 Type III (quit/ retire) Employment Adaptation ............. 154 Time 2 ........................................ 155 All Adaptation Types ............................ 155 Compatibility with the Conceptual Model ................... 155 Interaction of the Variable Groups ....................... 159 Conclusions ....................................... 163 Implications ....................................... 170 Implications for Practice ............................. 170 Implications for Policy .............................. 173 Implications for Further Research ....................... 175 LIST OF REFERENCES ................................. 177 APPENDD( A: Instrument ................................ 188 APPENDD( B: Correlation Matrix ............................ 203 APPENDDI C: Means for Interval Variables ..................... 205 APPENDIX D: UCRIHS Approval Letter ....................... 206 Table I Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Table 15 Table 1 Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Table 12 LIST OF TABLES Probability of Time 1, Type I Influencing Time 2, Type II and Type 111 Employment Adaptations Correlations of Selected Caregiver/ Care Recipient Characteris- tics with Employment Adaptation Time 1 Oneway AN OVA by Type I for Selected Care- giver/Care Recipient Characteristics Time 2 Oneway ANOVA by Type I for Selected Care- giver/Care Recipient Characteristics Contingency Tables by Type II and Type III for Selected Caregiver/Care Recipient Characteristics Time 1 Regression of Significant Caregiver/Care Recipient Characteristics and Type I Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Functions for Selected Significant Caregiver/ Care Recipient Characteristics and Time 1, Type II Adaptation Discriminant Function Classification for Selected Significant Caregiver/ Care Recipient Characteristics and Time 1, Type II Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Selected Significant Caregiver/Care Recipient Characteristics and Time 1, Type II Adaptation Independent t-test for Caregiver Household Income and Time 1, Type 111 Employment Adaptation Independent t-test for Caregiver Education and Time 2, Type III Employment Adaptation Correlations of Family Support Variables with Employment Adaptation xi 100 102 103 104 105 112 113 114 114 115 115 119 Table 13. Table 14 Table l5 Table 16 Table 17 Table l8 Table 19 Table 20 Table 21 Table 22 Table 13 Table 14 Table 15 Table 16 Table 17 Table 18 Table 19 Table 20 Table 21 Table 22 Table 23 Table 24 Table 25 Table 26 xii Regression for Family Support Variables and Time 1, Type I Employment Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Functions for Family Support Vari- ables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adaptations Discriminant Function Classification Results for Family Support Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adapta- tions Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Family Support Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type HI Employ- ment Adaptations Correlations of Caregiver Involvement with Employment Adaptation Regression for Caregiver Involvement and Time 1, Type I Employment Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Functions for Caregiver Involvement Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adaptations Discriminant Function Classification for Caregiver Involvement Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adaptations Independent t-test for Hours of Supervision and Time 1, Type 111 Employment Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Caregiver Involvement Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Employment Adaptations Regression of Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type I Adaptation Backward Regression of Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type I Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Functions for Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adaptations Discriminant Function Classification for Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adaptations 122 123 124 126 129 133 134 134 135 136 138 139 140 141 Table 2' Table 2i Table 29 Table 3( Table 27 Table 28 Table 29 Table 30 xiii Canonical Discriminant Functions for Selected Significant Variables and Time 2, Type III Adaptation Discriminant Function Classification for Selected Significant Variables and Time 2, Type HI Adaptation Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type II and Type III Adaptations Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Selected Significant Variables and Time 2, Type III Adaptation 143 143 145 146 Figure 1 Figure I Figure 3 Figure 4 Figure 5 Figure ( Figure 7 Figure 8 Figure 9 Figure 1 Figure 2 Figure 3 Figure 4 Figure 5 Figure 6 Figure 7 Figure 8 Figure 9 LIST OF FIGURES Preliminary Conceptual Model for Family Eldercare and Employment Adaptations Independent and Dependent Variables Caregiver Characteristics Time 2 Employment Status Changes (N = 1 l9) Frequencies for Employment Adaptations Disruption of Normal Work and Daily Activities because of Caregiving Activities Means for Caregiver Involvement Variables Transitional Conceptual Model for Family Eldercare & Employment Adaptations Concluding Conceptual Model for Family Eldercare & Employment Adaptations xiv 38 40 68 95 97 127 130 158 162 CHAPTER I Introduction The expanded pool of employees with eldercare obligations solicits increasing concern in corporate America (Scharlach, Lowe, & Schneider, 1991). The relationship between caregiving responsibility and work quality and productivity generates the most employer interest. The escalating numbers of employed caregivers of elderly family members prompt predictions that eldercare will be the benefit issue of the 1990’s (Denton, Love, & Slate, 1990; Friedman, 1986; Kola & Dunkle, 1988). The eldercare phenomenon parallels the emergence of childcare as a benefit issue thirty years ago. Warshaw, Barr, Rayman, Schachter, and Lucas (1986) state that in the sixties, companies were forced to consider employer-supported childcare due to the influx of women into the work force. They further maintain that the workplace has paid little attention to the issue of employees with the responsibility for incapacitated or disabled family members. Today, childcare remains a problem; however, the eldercare concern may rival or surpass the childcare phenomenon. What makes the issue of eldercare and the workplace so significant today? An examination of converging demographic and social trends provides insight into the situation. The trends underscore the importance of investigating the influence of acquiring the family eldercare role on female employment adaptation. Demographic changes, the shifting role of women, and the phenomenon of family caregiving merge to cballe increasin; persons i The pop Waldo. of Amer be 17.3‘ filming 2 to challenge corporate ability to maintain a competitive edge through fully productive employees. Scope of the Problem Changing population demographics activate this issue. The eldest cohort is increasing rapidly while the youngest cohort is decreasing. The percentage of elderly persons in the United States will swell from approximately 12 % in 1990 to 21% by 2030. The population 19 years or less, now 29% of the total, will decline to 23.5% by 2030 (Waldo, Sonnefeld, McKusick, & Amett III, 1989). Haber (1989) illustrates the aging of America at a slightly slower pace by projecting the percentage of persons over 65 to be 17.3% in 2020 and 21.7% in 2050. Within that group, persons over 85 are the fastest growing segment and are frequently dependent on long-term care, usually provided by their families. In the past century, many changes contributed to the population shift toward longevity, increased numbers of elderly persons, and declining numbers of younger people. Hooyman and Ryan (1987) refer to changes in mortality and fertility rates and altered migration patterns as possible reasons for the dramatic increase in the absolute number and proportion of older people. Improved preventive medical care and the actual care and technology connected with acute illness episodes are other explanations for longer life expectancies. This, however, is not without cost; those improvements in preventive and acute medical care increase the possibility of chronic disease. The incidence of chronic illness increases with age and the presence of chronic diseases escalate the likelihood of dependency on others. The National Council on Aging 3 estimates 6.6 million people over 65 need some physical assistance. They predict that number will increase to nine million by 2000 and 19 million by 2040 (RNA, #21, 1989). Thus, more elderly persons and the increased incidence of chronic disease create challenges for families. Brody and Brody (1989) maintain that thirty years of research disprove the wide- spread myth that families do not provide eldercare. In fact, they contend that families always have been and continue to be the "main provider of long-term health and social support to the aged" (p. 259). Female family members usually meet the caregiving challenge. In the Travelers survey, 63% of primary care providers were female (The Travelers Companies, 1988). Stone, Cafferata, and Sangl (1987) report that 72% of the caregivers identified through the 1982 National Long-Tenn Care Survey and Informal Caregivers Survey were female. A progress report for Caregiver II (core study for this research) indicates that 76.3% of the caregivers were women (Pohl, Given, & Given, 1991). Other research also documents that women provide the most eldercare (Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, & Schoonover, 1987; Brody & Schoonover, 1986; Horowitz, 1985). Women working outside the home in record numbers complicate this issue. In 1950, about 33% of women, 16-64 years of age and from the civilian, non-institutional- ized population, were in the labor force, compared to 64% in 1985 (U .8. Bureau of the Census, 1986). The Bureau (1986) notes a decline (91% to 85%) for men during the same period and predicts that women will comprise 46% of the labor force in 1995. The annual growth rate for employed women between the years 1975-1990 was 2.8%; however. (Fulleno: VT eipzuidir.T employed rate of ind 54 worked 1991). Fu continue to wOmen beti Pres carEgll'ing ‘ Depmmem 1!}ch SUl force, and characleristi employees, i and (d) On ll A n3 Retired PeTS< WETC WQmEn‘. llallof [be We 4 however, that percentage is predicted to taper to 1.6% during the years 1990-2005 (Fullerton, 1991). Winfield (1988) maintains that women have been the dominant factor in an expanding labor force. Brody (1990) concurs and reports that the proportion of employed women rose from 24% in 1930 to 70% in 1985. She claims the most rapid rate of increase is with middle-aged women. In 1990, 71% of women from age 45 to 54 worked outside the home, representing an increase from 54.6% in 1975 (Fullerton, 1991). Fullerton predicts that the percentage of employed women in that age group will continue to climb, increasing to 81.5% by 2005. Creedon (1988) estimates that 75% of women between 45 and 60 will be in the work force by the year 2000. Presumably, the number of women coping with the dual roles of work and caregiving will continue an upward climb. Scharlach (1989), using 1986 United States Department of Labor information, asserts that the number of employed caregivers will increase substantially due to the increased number of women workers, an aging work force, and increased numbers of elders needing assistance. He also notes general characteristics of the employed caregiver. They are: (a) usually older than other employees, (b) generally in their 40’s or 50’s, (c) equally represented in job categories, and (d) on the job longer. A national survey of caregivers, conducted for the American Association of Retired Persons and the Travelers Companies Foundation, found that 75 % of caregivers were women, with an average age of 45 years (The Travelers Companies, 1988). Over half of the women were employed, with about forty-two percent reporting full-time work and 13% claiming part-time employment. The remaining women were either not employee considere. years old. Wl the 45-64 that group for wome: With chilc Ho care, She homemakz: lime, SCH Slgnlllcani “Thin fOr Wm 5 employed or homemakers (27%) or retired (16%). Sixty-three percent of the women considered themselves to be primary caregivers of a disabled family member at least 50 years old. Wisensale and Allison (1988) add to this finding when they report that women in the 45—64 age group are most likely to provide care to a disabled parent or husband. Of that group, 65% of women aged 45-54 work; however, that number decreases to 42% for women aged 55-64. Caregiving is a life cycle event for many women, beginning with childcare and ending with eldercare. Horowitz (1985) notes a gender related division of labor for providing family care. She reports that the caregiving daughter usually holds primary responsibility for homemaking, child-rearing, emotional support for family members, and often works full- time. Several years later, the findings remain consistent. Finley (1989), too, describes significant gender differences in caregiving. She also notes that, in spite of more role conflict for women than for men, women are more likely to provide eldercare. Society seldom places the same multiple demands on men that it does on women. When both men and women provide care, the hours involved by gender varies. Anastas, Gibeau, and Larson (1990) report the mean hours of care for female caregivers is 10.9 per week while the average for their male counterparts drops to 5.8 hours per week. Results of the Travelers survey indicate even more disparity; 16.1 hours for women versus 5.3 hours for men (The Travelers Companies, 1988). High care hours have the potential to negatively influence the ability to maintain employment responsibili- ties. 6 Brody (1985) refers to the "woman in the middle,” a woman surrounded by multiple pressures. This woman has principal responsibility for the care of a dependent parent (usually the mother), experiences competing demands on time and energy, suffers strains as a result of parent care, possibly has the ”empty nest” refilled, and often works full or part-time. Sidel (1986, p. 167) echoes the dilemma of women in the middle and notes "younger women are increasingly torn among their responsibilities toward their own families, their responsibilities toward the older persons, and these days a job as well." A study by Young and Kahana (1989) illustrates employment as a significant predictor of adverse reaction during the caregiving period. The Older Women’s League publication, Failing America ’s Caregivers: A Status Report on Women Who Care (1989) states that 1.8 million women are simultaneously caring for children and elderly relatives. Half of these women are in the work force; 20 percent of the women have a parent living in their home. Obviously, substantial numbers of women provide care to an elderly family member while also attempting to maintain employment and manage additional family responsibilities. The number of eldercare hours provided varies with the characteristics of the care recipient, the involvement of other family members and friends, and the utilization of formal services. Scharlach (1989) reports that 20-28% of employees provide care at any one time and give 610 hours per week of aid to impaired elders. Results of the Travelers survey, where 20% of employees over age 30 provided an average of 10.2 care hours per week, support that statistic (The Travelers Companies, 1988). Gibeau (1988) notes the primary caregiver provides 12.1 hours of care per week; a slightly higher mean than the preceding references. ll} some car: caregivers job! In ar Connectic spent an a Bit ry insight First. they aid and d conflicted decreasing TOUTS for 1 lladi’uonal [0 {huge Wt Wk Cle rehoned '1, Th disCOl'ey-ed one and a Alfiqough illiimncC 7 While reports of means are useful, they also obscure the intense involvement some caregivers contribute. For example, in the Travelers survey, eight percent of all caregivers spent more than 35 hours per week providing care; almost a second full-time job! In another example of high care hours, Wisensale and Allison (1986) refer to a Connecticut survey where most caregivers were females between 40-59 years of age who spent an average of 82 hours monthly (20.5 hours/week) providing care. Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, and Schoonover (1987) provide complementa- ry insight into the impact of caregiving hours per week on the employed caregiver. First, they refer to the persevering worker as one who provides 12.7 hours per week of aid and does not consider quitting her job to do so. The second employee example, a conflicted worker, provides 23.3 care hours per week and is considering either decreasing work hours or stopping employment. Of interest, the number of caregiving hours for the conflicted worker is just under the 24.1 care hours per week that the traditional non-working caregiver provides! Finally, the third employee category refers to those who quit work to provide care; these caregivers give about 38 hours of help per week. Clearly, much care hour variation exists from the mean of 10-12 hours per week reported in some studies. This variance partially relates to the dependencies of the elder. Scharlach (1989) discovered that employees who cared for cognitively impaired elders spent approximately one and a half times as many hours in caregiving as did other employee caregivers. Although ethnicity appears to create diversity, there is limited literature reporting ethnic differences in caregiving. Most inferences arise from case experience and indirect sources of data (Sakauye, 1988). White-Means and Thornton (1990) examined data from the 1982 .‘ to study A hmdmm descent to of English of care per Tn; or ethnic restrictions experience 139011 that demands. Ho necessam} daugluers . Caregiver g 8 the 1982 National Long-Term Care Survey and National Survey of Informal Caregivers to study Americans with different ethnic backgrounds in relation to caregiving. They found that the mean hours of care per day ranged from 2.45 for caregivers of German descent to 4.20 for those of African origin (17.15 to 29.40 hours per week). Caregivers of English extraction provided 2.85 hours while those of Irish lineage gave 2.94 hours of care per day (19.95 to 20.58 hours per week) to their ailing elders. The hours of care provided create costs for caregivers, regardless of employment or ethnic status. Horowitz (1985) and Buglass (1989) report that emotional strain, restrictions on time and freedom, and economic hardships generate the burdens experienced by some caregivers. Hooyman and Ryan (1987) reinforce this finding and report that many women are physically and emotionally exhausted by multiple role demands. However, choosing to leave the work force in order to provide care does not necessarily decrease burden. Barnes, Given, and Given (1991) report that adult daughters who ended employment in order to give care evidenced the greatest nwd for caregiver support and were at increased risk for depression. Clearly, multiple issues surround the caregiving experience. Statement of the Problem The growing population of employed female family caregivers and the possible negative interactions between caregiving and employment responsibilities stimulate increasing concern. The long-held notion that work and family denote two separate worlds has been under intense bombardment and is weakening under the pressures of a TWO°g€lit and life lbce, 19 family, b interactio Purpose 9 two-gender work force (Winfield, 1988). The increased flexibility of former rigid role and life patterns heightens anxiety about the interaction of work and family (Frone & Rice, 1987). Managers are beginning to see the close connection between work and family, but its influence is unknown. The present study addressed this family and work interaction. Purpose of the Study The purpose of the study was to investigate the influence of acquiring the role of caregiver for a physically disabled elderly relative on the employment adaptation of women. Three types of adaptation were considered in the study. Type I adaptation involves maintaining, but altering one’s work role and includes: (a) arriving late/leaving early, (b) missing work without pay, (c) taking sick/personal days, ((1) changing work hours, (e) refusing jobs or promotions, (t) not looking for jobs or better jobs, and (g) other ways that caregiving may affect work. Type II adaptation is taking a leave of absence and Type III adaptation is quitting or retiring from employment in order to provide care. Unless specified otherwise, the study objectives encompassed each type of adaptation. The study included examination of (a) the immediate and delayed (after three months) employment adaptation for the female employee; (b) the influence of selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics (caregiver age, relationship to care recipient, marital status, education, occupation, household income, employment status, co-residence status, and care recipient age and gender) on employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2; (c) the influence of family support (number in network, amount of help, and frequency of familj Time 2; of daily activities Managing researchers Mugging The extensi. SCH'iceS N The The firs: l Caregmng‘ Provide Ca- meet their the Wes 0 10 of family assistance with eldercare) on caregiver employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2; and (d) the influence of caregiver involvement (total assistance with activities of daily living (ADL), instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), and health care activities (HCA) and the hours of both physical care and supervision) on employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2; and (e) the probability of the caregiver displaying Type H or Type HI adaptation at Time 2 given the level of Type I adaptation at Time 1. Relationship of this Study to Grant #2 R01 AGO6584 This study utilized data from the core research effort, "Caregiver Responses to Managing Elderly Patients at Home" (Caregiver II). Caregiver H, conducted by researchers at Michigan State University, is an extension of ”Caregiver Responses to Managing Elderly Patients at Home" (Caregiver I). The original study began in 1986; the extension (Caregiver II) continues through 1992 and is funded by Health and Human Services National Institute on Aging. The core research that forms the basis for the present study focused on two goals. The first was to identify an inception cohort of family members who were new to caregiving, to follow them for 18 months, and to compare those who continued to provide care with those who ceased their caregiving role and chose other alternatives to meet their elderly relative’s needs. The second goal was to describe, over 18 months, the types of care provided to all patients whose caregivers either ceased or persisted in family caregiving (Pohl, Given, and Given, 1991). Significan As increase (1. job perior: financial b morivate er Will persis liability ar, “&y lou‘hich ti... FUpoauor adaquately ~ l 1 Significance and Generalizability As noted, the number of employed female family caregivers will continue to increase due to current and projected demographic and social trends. The employees’ job performance, time lost from work, and the ability to remain employed influence the financial bottom line for employers (Scharlach, 1989). Consequently, the desire to motivate employees, decrease absenteeism and tardiness, and attract productive workers will persist in generating employer concern. Increased productivity and financial viability are employers’ desired outcomes. Warshaw, Barr, Rayman, Schachter, and Lucas (1986) maintain that the ”extent to which the competing demands of work and family care may actually inhibit labor force participation or affect either the quality of family care or job performance has not been adequately explored” (p. 2). Also, Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, and Schoonover (1987) claim that little research exists regarding patterns of women and work in relation to eldercare. Most previous research uses the context of childcare (Friedman, 1988). Kola and Dunkle (1988) also note the lack of adequate research focusing on the interaction between family and work roles. They, too, maintain that most existing research about family and work relates to childcare. The present study adds to that body of knowledge by examining the influence of acquiring the family caregiver role on female employment adaptation, both at the inception of the eldercare function and three months later. Much previous research involving family caregivers studied caregiver burden (Archbold, 1983; Barusch & Spaid, 1989; Brody, 1985; Fitting, Rabins, Lucas, & Eastham, 1986; George & Gwyther, 1986; Gilhooly, 1984; Given, King, Collins, & 12 Given, 1988; Given, Stommel, Collins, King, & Given, 1990; Horowitz, 1985; Montgomery, 1989; Stommel, Given, & Given, 1990). A gradual shift in research emphasis from caregiver burden to the influence of eldercare on employment began about six years ago. While that body of literature continues to grow and provide valuable insight into the complicated interaction of work and caregiving, much is still unknown. Some people choose to stop work in order to provide care; others elect to halt informal caregiving and institutionalize the elderly relative in order to continue employment. In between those extreme adaptations, a range of alternative behaviors, such as decreased work hours, choosing a less demanding job, or arranging to share the care with formal and informal caregivers, exists. It is important to identify key events that influence these decisions. In contrast to the present study, most previous researchers investigating the consequences of caregiving studied well-established caregivers and used a cross-sectional design. However, some longitudinal studies exist and they present a complex picture of change over time (Given, Stommel, & Lin, 1991; Robinson & Thumher, 1979; Stoller & Pugliesi, 1989b; Zarit, Todd, & Zarit, 1986). Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, and Boise (1990) urge that future research on employees and caregiving responsibilities considers variation within and across groups of caregivers as well as how caregiving demands (and influence on employment adaptation) change over time. Stone and Short (1990) reference a growing number of federal, state, and private initiatives that target benefits to employed caregivers. They, too, recommend the empiric investigation of the interrelationship with caregiving and employment. Conceptual F Maudie. The cr general theori unmet relational con‘ Adaptation of FTSPOCtit-e. ent'ironment i analytically 5i biological en. In the Changing ne disequiitbnu, Eldercare. T cmplol'lllent The 2 it“ famineE (Blister, 19g “menses fr other The family c 13 Conceptual Framework Way—”meant The conceptual framework and theoretical base for the study flow from two general theories; namely, the human ecological and choice and exchange theories. The use of these theories allows the examination of family caregiving and employment in a relational context. Caregiving represents a dynamic, not static process of interaction and adaptation of family members with their environment, a basic tenet of the ecological perspective. The individual (caregiver) or unit (family) in interaction with the environment constitutes an ecosystem. The family ecosystem environment includes the analytically separated, but interrelated human-built, social-culture, and natural physical- biological environments (Bubolz & Sontag, in press). In the context of their environments, individuals and families adapt to meet changing needs. Adaptation allows individuals and families to respond to the disequilibrium that may occur when the family system is disturbed by the need to provide eldercare. The primary caregiver endeavors to maintain a balance between home and employment obligations. The family as a whole responds to the altered situation. The adaptation (modification of behavior, feelings, ideas, and the environment), that families make to meet changed member demands is a key ecological concept (Bristor, 1990). A primary need of one person (in the present study, eldercare) triggers responses from other relatives. The employment adaptation that caregivers may make in order to satisfy eldercare commitments is the dependent variable for the study. Other ecological concepts important to this study include the family and resources. The family can be a resource to the employed caregiver. Matthews and Rosner (1988) report that 5 their parent of the prin resources to tics also ma the present 1 age, relatio income. am of CO’TCSldl Characterist Thr the Priman are involve and Three r Care, Term of the car e mi eco Co: geographic family rel themselves and flimit l Thrill-jam;11 14 report that sister dyads used routine involvement and back-up support in order to meet their parent care obligations. The responses of the family, either the direct involvement of the primary caregiver or the family assistance other members provide, denote resources for the elderly relative needing help. Caregiver and care recipient characteris- tics also may be resources that influence the caregiving and employment interaction. In the present study, selected caregiver/care recipient characteristics include the caregiver’s age, relationship to the care recipient, marital status, education, occupation, household income, and employment status; the care recipient’s age and gender; and the dyad feature of co-residence. Family support, caregiver involvement, and caregiver/care recipient characteristics are the independent variables in the study. Throughout the process of eldercare and the probable employment adaptation by the primary caregiver, other meaningful ecological concepts (time, space, and energy) are involved. In the present study, the period between the inception of the caregiver role and three months later represents an actual example of the calendar time spent providing care. Temporal orientations, such as past experiences, present needs, and future desires of the caregiver and family system influenced the decisions (made in the context of the family ecosystem environment) that led to the acquisition of the eldercare role. Concurrently, space, such as the residence the caregiver dyad may share or the geographic distance between members in the family network, impacts eldercare and family relations. Also, conceptual space, as with family members ”distancing" themselves from direct care involvement or the bounded, but interrelated systems of work and family influence the care situation. Likewise, energy, or the ebb and flow of individual and family internal and external resources, enables the adaptation and response to employmen support may employment a Choice and E. The in Competing det discussing the numbers of ch (or least loss ) choices regarc decrease Worl dim Choices Additional ele calegiying 103 Finally dsisranCe to t DTlmay-y 93mg 15 to employment and family needs that regulate system equilibrium. For example, family support may allow the caregiver to recharge personal energy levels and minimize employment adaptation. W The interaction and adaptation families make as they struggle to manage the competing demands of work and eldercare involve many decisions or choices. When discussing the choice and exchange theory, Nye (1979) maintains that one makes infinite numbers of choices in order to decrease costs and maximize rewards for the most profit (or least loss). He views choice as the most significant aspect of the theory. Some choices regarding caregiving and work are obvious and individual, such as choosing to decrease work hours in order to provide care. Caregivers also make less obvious or direct choices, for example, using vacation time to meet eldercare obligations. Additional elements of preference for the family caregiver may include the amount of caregiving load assumed and the time allotted for competing work and family roles. Finally, other family members choose to provide either physical or emotional assistance to the caregiver or the care recipient or to distance themselves from the care situation. Thus, decisions regarding eldercare that the family makes may influence the primary caregiver’s adaptation to employment. Assimpm Assumptio. The follov l. The 2. The neer 3. All TCSC em- 5. 1:3r me- hu; in 16 Annotations Assumptions of the Ecological Perspective The following assumptions are from Bubolz and Sontag (in press). 1. 2. 10. ll. 12. The family and its interaction with the environment is considered an ecosystem.* The family manages the bio-physical, psycho-social, economic, and nurturance needs and functions of its members.* All humans are interdependent with one another and with environmental resources.* The properties, structure, and processes involved with the family and its environment must be considered as interdependent and analyzed as a system. Families are interdependent with other forms of life and the non-living environ- ment. Adaptation to their environments is a continuous process in families. All parts of the environment (natural biological-physical, social—cultural, and human built) are interrelated and influence each other. Families are part of and interact with many environments. Families are energy transformation systems that use energy for survival, interaction, and adaptation. Family interactions are guided by physical and biological laws of nature and human derived rules. Environments provide limitations and opportunities for families. Families have varied amounts of control related to environmental interactions. 13. D1 ’=key Assump The fol h.) «4351” ft.) 13. 17 Decision-making is the central control process that families use to attain individual and family goals. * = key assumptions Assumptions of Choice and Exchange Theory The following assumptions are from Nye (1979). 1. Human behavior is rational in spite of the occasional use of insufficient informa- tion and incorrect forecasting of the future. Humans at any level (from individual to nations) act to decrease their costs and increase their rewards. Humans are able to expect increased rewards and decreased costs from responsive governmental or private institutions. Humans are able to presume general reciprocity between themselves and society. Humans recognize their choices influence the rewards and costs of others in the groups to which they belong. Assumptions of this Study 1. The ability of an elderly individual to adapt to increased functional health problems relates to the amount and kind of family resources available. Families make choices to aid their elderly relative based on a consideration of alternatives, costs, and benefits. Families prefer to help their elderly member live independently and avoid institutionalization. 4. The. adapt i Thes ande 6. 'The shua Lhnhathor Tin them to de ha150 see the Origin; is not the lisearcltt II the on'gi‘ “EVE of We at. enigma lite Emm Unknowl SimPath 18 4. The degree of caregiver involvement in eldercare influences employment adaptation. 5. The size and quality of the caregiver’s family network influence the care situation and employment adaptation. 6. The assistance families provide to the primary caregiver influences the care situation and employment adaptation. Limitations The core study purpose is to identify an inception cohort of caregivers and follow them to determine differences between those who cease caregiving and those who persist. It also seeks to describe the type of care provided to all patients regardless of whether the original caregiver stopped or continued to provide care. Because the core study focus is not the employment adaptation of female caregivers, it is impossible for the present research to provide an exhaustive investigation of the caregiving/work interaction. The use of secondary data limits the variables available for analysis to those from the original research. In some instances, the same variable is not measured on each wave of data, thus making comparisons of two time frames difficult or impossible. While the employment variables for this study are valuable, additional components would enhance the usefulness of the results. For example, the general work environment and the employment policies and practices affecting eldercare for caregivers in this study are unknown. Perhaps some caregivers in the sample are employed by companies sympathetic to their employees’ family care needs while others are not. Objective measures of self-report in Also. thus, the ma; be measured ployod careg 1990). As \1 family supp measures. 1 arbount and their own e1 ‘ the Elationg‘ Final to erhillcity, ushhhl does late is COnsi. ofcahegll’er hhhhlllace a“ 19 measures of the actual employment adaptations used (employment records, rather than self-report information) would increase the value of the present research. Also, the analysis did not include comparison groups of employed non-caregivers; thus, the magnitude of the influence of caregiving on employment adaptation could not be measured. Systematic research on the differences between employed and nonem- ployed caregivers needs to be done (Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, & Boise, 1990). As with employment adaptation data, the caregiver self-report responses for family support or eldercare involvement offered subjective, rather than objective measures. Individuals vary in their perceptions of reality; thus, their judgments of the amount and frequency of family help may reflect those biases. Similarly, how they rate their own eldercare involvement may be influenced by their perception of the quality of the relationship with the care recipient or by their definition of the role. Finally, little research on eldercare and employment considers variables related to ethnicity. Thus, the analysis of caregiving determinants and labor market decisions usually does not use an ethnic approach (White-Means & Thornton, 1990). Although race is considered in the core study, preliminary information indicates the vast majority of caregiver dyads in the study are white. Ethnicity and its relationship to eldercare and workplace adaptation should be considered in the future. Rese the influem followed b exchange ll Influence Far researcher CHAPTER II Review of Literature Research and related literature are organized in five categories. Reviews include the influence of eldercare on employees and families, on employers, and on society, followed by an examination of the human ecological approach and the choice and exchange theory. Influence on Employees and Families Family caregiving’s effect on employees has stimulated increased attention from researchers. While studies generally support the notion that caregivers adapt employment to manage caregiving obligations, the reported amount of influence varies. Anastas, Gibeau, and Larson’s (1990) survey of employed caregivers was the first of its kind to look at working families and eldercare with a national rather than a regional perspective. Although not a large sample (N =409), results did support similar local or regional surveys. The respondents were rather evenly divided between male and female workers. In respOnse to questions about work adaptation because of caregiving, the caregivers in the study reported that they: (a) used vacation time—64% , (b) changed schedules—33%, (c) used personal leaves—32%, (d) missed work meetings—18%, (e) missed outside conferences-14%, (i) missed overtime—13%, (g) called in sick—8%, 20 (l1) refused mt their jobs—59 Anothr Persons and l were employ due to Careg benefits as a Other and Boyd (19 (M1557). hm only 49 respollsibilr'r 0f the other al'erage nor than fOI thc Fen emPIOyee S. m“ ('2 mlafionshjl dlingerences “leaving of Hugged WDmen Ve 21 (h) refused more responsible jobs—7%, (i) were unable to seek new jobs—6%, (i) left their jobs—5%, (k) refused job offers-4%, and (1) took leaves of absence—3%. Another study of caregivers (N=754) conducted by the Association for Retired Persons and The Travelers Companies Foundation (1988) found that 53% of caregivers were employed and that 38% of that group either lost time from work or came in late due to caregiving responsibilities. Twenty percent of the employed caregivers lost benefits as a result of the employment adaptation that they made. Other studies support the competing demands of work and caregiving. Scharlach and Boyd (1989) compared employed caregivers (N =34 1) with non-caregiving employees (N = 1557). They found that 73.1% of caregivers reported interference with their jobs, but only 49.1% of the other employees described conflicts with work and family responsibilities. The researchers also learned that 37% of the caregivers versus 23.2% of the other employees missed work due to family responsibilities. In addition, the average number of missed work days was one and one half times higher for caregivers than for those without care responsibility. Fernandez (1990) reports that eight percent of the people surveyed in his 1988 employee survey (N =26,000) recounted eldercare responsibilities while slightly over two percent (2.4%) claimed both elder and childcare obligations. Results about the relationship between caregiving responsibility and lost productive time show gender differences for caregivers of elderly persons, children, or both, and for those without caregiving responsibilities. In three of four categories, women had a higher percentage of missed work time. For example, for those with eldercare responsibilities, 45% of women versus 28% of men missed work; 33% of the women and 25% of men arrived late; and 54% percentages of = 81%; men The bi eldercare 0in work (womer left early (wo Noreen the g Nor surpnsir work adjustn grouping, Schar for Either Cg lmponam v; missed an a missed by c work Chang cognitively WW '1. versus 11% “I“ 53.19 Comm Erer 22 late; and 54% of the women versus 46% of men left early. However, nearly equal percentages of men and women reported dealing with family problems at work (women = 81%; men = 82%). The biggest gender differences came when caregivers claimed both child and eldercare obligations. Again, women reported greater disruption, especially with missed work (women = 55%; men = 28%), arrived late (women = 41%; men = 27%), and left early (women = 63%; men = 47%). Like with eldercare, there was little difference between the genders in dealing with care issues at work (women = 87%; men = 83%). Not surprisingly, those without care responsibility had little gender disparity in each work adjustment category, although women reported more disruption than men in each grouping. Scharlach’s (1989) study of TransAmerica Life employees who were caregivers for either cognitively or physically impaired elderly persons (N =332) also suggests that important work-related adaptation occurs. Caregivers of a cognitively impaired person missed an average of 3.4 hours of work in the previous month compared to 2.4 hours missed by caretakers of the physically impaired. Also, the percentage of those reporting work changes in the previous two months was substantially more for the caregiver of the cognitively impaired. For example, 41.8% of caregivers for the cognitively impaired reported ”left early" versus 28.8% of other caregivers; "extended a break" was 23.5% versus 11%; "took a day off (with pay) " was 45.5% versus 28.2%; "used vacation time” was 53.1% versus 36.8%; "too tired to work" was 42.4% versus 26.3%; and "considered quitting" was 14.7% versus 5.8%. I employr when th Roberts ofthec in three Suggest translat 1933). for fen Female Comte; non.“ in Poor EOOd h, 23 Another report involving TransAmerica employees (N =34l) indicates that employment adaptation was greatest when the care recipient was more impaired and when the caregiver felt that support for the role was inadequate (Scharlach, Sobel, & Roberts, 1991). That study emphasizes the importance of family roles and characteristics of the caregiver and the care recipient in predicting employment outcomes. Employment outcomes do make a difference! TransAmerica Life found that one in three TransAmerica employees providing eldercare missed work. Survey results suggest that Transamerica employees lose more than 1600 work days annually, translating into a yearly loss of $250,000 in salaries and benefits (”1,600 Days Go,” 1988). In another study (N =491), Stone and Short (1990) found an increased probability for female employees to adapt work schedules to meet caregiving responsibilities. Female caregivers were 11.8 % more likely to adjust their work schedules than their male counterparts. Also, white caregivers were 15.1% more likely to adjust schedules than non-white caretakers. Caregiver health complicated the work/ family conflict. Caregivers in poor health were more likely to adjust their work schedules than those who reported good health. Stone and Short (1990) also found that the higher the nwd of the elder, the more likely were caregivers to adjust their work schedules. For example, in their study, caregivers helping elders with behavioral problems were 18% more likely to alter their work schedules. Surprisingly, the severity of the care recipient’s activity of daily living dependencies did not influence caregiver work accommodation. However, 22-28% of caregivers worked with some employment adaptation. That percentage contrasts sharply with the Survey respons'r Chapmz work p moons higher “8833 than it “Wk 1 emplo been men 24 with the American Association of Retired Persons (1987) Caregivers in the Workplace Survey (N =322) where only five percent of caregivers reported their caregiving responsibilities regularly interfered with work duties. In a study (N =7801) of absenteeism and stress among employed caregivers, Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, and Boise (1990) conclude that interruptions during work provide the only measure of time loss obviously associated with caregiving responsibilities. However, in that study, interruption rates were two to three times higher for the caregiver employee than for workers without dependent responsibility. Again, women reported the most difficulty with work interruptions and, as expected, full- time employees had more problems than did the part-time group. The study also suggests that the personal and home-related aspects of the caregiver’s life suffered more than work-related components. Orodenker (1990) supports the notion of caregiving and work conflict and the resulting influence on employment adaptations. She reports that employed caregivers experienced more stress after they altered their work schedules because of their inability to balance competing role demands. The stress resulting from employment and caregiving conflicts may lead to the decision to quit work, especially for women. Data from the Family Survival Project suggest that caregiving is a stronger deterrent to adult daughters employment than to any other caregiver group (Enright & Friss, 1987). A later account notes that 22% of unemployed caregiving daughters in the study of brain-impaired adults quit their position in order to provide care (Enright, 1991). Anastas, Gibeau, and Larson (1990) also recount that women are more likely than men to describe high degrees of work and family conflict and to consider ending emplc pron] we ; likel) hour: stud} prov worn likel malr sens Inn: for ‘ “Urn. 25 employment. In their study (N =436), 75% of those who pondered quitting in order to provide care were women. Personal characteristics of the caregiver and the amount of care given are important factors in the decision equation. Relinquishing a job is more likely to be the choice of a woman over 50 who is also providing a large number of care hours per week (Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, & Schoonover, 1987). In their study of adult caregiving daughters (N = 150), 45% of the women who quit work provided 40 hours of care per week for their dependent mothers. These also were women with the lowest education, income, and occupational status and who were more likely to have a "job" versus a "career." Ferree (1987) supports that finding and maintains that "working-class women are not committed to careers in the middle-class sense, and they are likely to change jobs, refuse promotions, and restrict hours in order to carry out their domestic responsibilities” (p. 292). Finally, preliminary results from the analysis of data from the present study sample showed that caregivers made some employment adaptation in order to provide care (Franklin, 1991). At Time 1, their involvement with family eldercare was significantly correlated with missing work days without pay, decreasing total work hours, taking a leave of absence, or quitting work. However, there were no significant correlations with adaptation in employment hours (arriving late/leaving early). Influence on the Employer Employee caregiving responsibilities influence the workplace in differing ways for employers. Key costs to employers include productivity losses due to increased numbers of personal phone calls made or received by employees and more time off due to illness. '. the employe e1dercare(' valued emp‘ Corr intense cor. implication to attract a influence c benefits (N have recog Travelers] Lowe, & S 1983); Ae (Femande; The [0 Urese a Schneideu per year fo; ml for Cac Cos mlSSed 0r ‘ 26 to illness. The elevated employer health benefit expenses stemming from a decline in the employee’s health that began with caregiving responsibilities increase the cost of eldercare ("Eldercare Benefits," 1988). Fernandez (1990) adds the ultimate loss of a valued employee to the employer’s costs. Companies with a large, middle-aged, female work force and that also experience intense competition for certain job skills often become acutely aware of eldercare implications. Without accommodation to family nwds, some companies lose their ability to attract and retain workers (Friedman & Gray, 1989). Employers who see the influence of family caregiving on productivity and quality tend to support eldercare benefits (Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, & Boise; 1990). Organizations that have recognized and responded to eldercare’s significance for their company include Travelers Insurance Companies, Bank of America, IBM, and Phillip Morris (Scharlach, Lowe, & Schneider, 1991); Southwestern Bell and Remington Products (Kola & Dunkle, 1988); Aetna Life and Casualty, Marriott (Buglass, 1989); and Chase Manhattan (Fernandez, 1990). The desire for increased productivity and cost savings provides a strong impetus to these and other companies concerned about eldercare. Scharlach, Lowe, and Schneider (1991) estimate that a company without formal eldercare programs loses $2500 per year for each employee with eldercare responsibilities. Merck and Company estimate that for each dollar spent on family care, it gets three returned (Fernandez, 1990). Costs to the employer may be directly related to the employed caregiver and missed or poor quality work or indirectly associated by the low morale that may ultimately permeate employees who must fill the gap left by another worker’s absence in poor p relationshij mmfleon annoyed a anoneenr generate 1: end as pe billion do] work due Replacem. SOme ofn bUdget In employer: 27 or poor performance. While an element of common sense exists regarding the relationship of predictable attendance and productivity, clear outcomes of stress and morale on productivity are more obscure (Friedman, 1988). A co-worker may be annoyed at having to work harder to cover a caregiver’s absence, even though the absenteeism is explained. Employee peer resentment and unhappiness may result and generate less total productivity. Also, caregiver absences that begin as temporary may end as permanent, thus creating additional employer costs. It is estimated that 100 billion dollars are lost annually when trained and experienced workers are absent from work due to their own or family illness (Bureau of National Affairs, #21, 1989). Replacement of workers, whether for a temporary need or a permanent change, creates some of those costs. Orientation and training expenses profoundly affect an employer’s budget. In spite of costs stemming from employees with eldercare responsibilities, some employers do not see a problem. A survey of 96 New York companies indicated that over 50% of the companies did not see employee caregivers as presenting work-related problems (Warshaw, Barr, Rayman, Schachter, & Lucas, 1986). However, the researchers ultimately concluded those issues may be hidden and, instead, more acceptable problems brought to the workplace. A similar exploratory study by Kola and Dunkle (1988) found that more than half of the surveyed organizations indicated that caregivers’ needs seldom came to the workplace. However, the remaining respondents indicated that many of their employees needed time off for eldercare that was manifested in lateness, absenteeism, and emergency hours and unscheduled time off. The same employment issues also were nnpo: inthe atnor ofhu Conrr Care} famn olrh is an the e “arrn ‘0 UM Liven 28 important for the companies in the New York study who did see eldercare as a problem in the workplace (Warshaw, Barr, Rayman, Schlachter, & Lucas, 1986). Even businesses that recognize eldercare’s importance have done little to take action. A survey of 101 companies initiated by Personnel Journal indicated that 67% of human resource executives felt that eldercare should be considered, but that only about 10% actively studied the problem (Magnus, 1988). More significant, the vast majority (89%) of the executives said they have no plans to address the issue. Only three of the 101 companies have a benefit that supports eldercare. Influence on Society Jacobs (1987, p. 18) defines social cost as the ". ..measure of the total resource commitment made by all members of the society in the undertaking of any activity. " Caregiver employment adaptation influences personal resources available, as well as family, co-worker, employer, and societal reactions. Graaff (1990) advises clarification of the scope of society when discussing social cost. For example, in this study, society is the United States, not the world community, although as ecological theory suggests, the effects of the interactions of family caregiving and employment cannot be confined to this country. Diverse repercussions affect multiple levels, from the micro (individual) to the macro (world) and the reverse. The costs of employed caregivers on society are often hidden. Some burdens of working caregivers, such as the influence of caregiving on health, schedules, and esteem, can affect attendance and productivity. If employment adaptation necessary to provide family care leads to lost wages, a decrease in buying power and tax contribution results. Also, a health 1 stemmi also suf Short ( gross r suppor- benefit COHSllll 29 Also, a possible increase in public assistance (more caregiver unemployment) and higher health care costs (increased caregiver morbidity) may occur (Horowitz, 1985). Costs stemming from the morbidity of the caregiver may escalate, particularly if the caregiver also suffers from chronic ill health that worsens with the strain of caregiving. Stone and Short (1990) support Horowitz’ findings. They regard societal costs as a decrease in the gross national product, foregone income tax revenue, and increased expenditures to support caregivers with decreased incomes, fringe benefits, and future retirement benefits. These costs ultimately result in a higher price of goods and services for the consumer. Of course, the decreased expense of informal care provided by families versus the price of formal care available in institutions somewhat offsets this cost. A study by Hu, Huang, and Cartwright (1986) indicates average annual nursing home charges to be $22,458, while the yearly homecare cost mean was $11,735. Feldstein (1988) estimates the average annual nursing home expenditure to be $27,000. Either institutional cost figure suggests that it is cheaper to provide care at home. Limited private coverage for nursing home care adds to the cost for society. Government, usually in the form of Medicaid, covers the expense when families become impoverished. When government pays, society pays in the form of higher taxes. Conceptual Framework 1 i A r a Human ecology theory assumes that phenomena must be examined in the wholeness of interaction and interdependence rather than with simple cause-and-effect nhnm mane study, be fai. rxnne ulnn This 11 used intern Ship e cowc ”16 ex Wmn men elihEr nmnr hUma Dim-*1 em")! 510k) 30 relationships (Andrews, Bubolz, & Paolucci, 1980). This allows analysis and interventions at several levels, depending on the researcher’s area of interest. In this study, the interaction of work and family roles provides the focus. This interaction may be fairly simple, with minimal repercussions, or more complex, with effects that permeate through multiple levels and environments. Bronfenbrenner (1979) proposed a nested arrangement of concentric environmen- tal structures; the microsystem, the mesosystem, the exosystem, and the macrosystem. This idea clarifies the importance of work and family interactions. While the employed caregiver functions at the micro level within either the home or work environments, the interrelationship between work and family comprises the mesosystem. This interrelation- ship extends to affect events in the work setting that, in turn, influence the reactions of co-workers. This indirect result, involving co-workers (peers or supervisors) illustrates the exosystem. Finally, the macro or largest system includes society as a whole. Thus, what occurs at the micro level ultimately extends to involve the macrosystem. The reverse of this process also is true. For example, public policy related to the support of either the family caregiver or the frail elderly person changes the care situation and its ramifications. The environments of a family ecosystem (the natural biological-physical, the human-built, and the social-cultural) as described by Bubolz and Sontag (in press) also provide the context for the varied interactions that families experience. These environments are analytically separate, but interrelated and impact each other throughout family caregiver and employment interaction and adaptation. Examples of the natural biological-physical environment include the climate, atmosphere, soil, trees, or what is commonly made to t1 goals or n houses (w hospitals ( 01 equipr presence ramificatr' lllSllllltlor commum “1 all em 31 commonly called ”nature," while the human-bth environment involves modifications made to the natural setting to enable survival (food or shelter) or to meet other human goals or nwds (family involvement, employment). These human alterations might be houses (with adjusted living quarters to meet eldercare needs), factories, businesses, or hospitals (to meet employment nwds), or material objects (household goods, belongings, or equipment). The third environmental component, social-cultural, includes the presence of humans and their interactions (families, friends, co-workers), cultural ramifications (societal norms, family values and support), social and economic institutions (regulatory systems and employment policies), and provides the basis for communication, order, and moral rules. Bubolz and Sontag (in press) assert that quality in all environments is necessary to maintain life. Employed female family caregivers make many decisions to maintain their lives and that of their elderly family member. These decisions, involving employment and eldercare responsibilities plus other personal and family obligations, are made in the context of the family ecosystem environment. According to Bristor (1990), the decision- making process used by humans begins with inputs (resources and information), continues through transactions (information processing), and ends with outputs (decisions, judgments, and actions). Characteristics of the interrelated family ecosystem environ- ments affect those choices or outcomes about employment and family. Kantor and Lehr’s (1975) dimensions of family process also contribute to the understanding of eldercare decisions. They (p. 36) describe dimensions as being "physical and conceptual fields of interactional activity" and refer to access dimensions of time, space, and energy, and target dimensions of affect, power, and meaning. These dimension for specif interfaces lhese inn A: includesl of elderc has for r individua example. £13111 de‘ ProceSs 32 dimensions provide a context for decision-making through which family members’ needs for specific goals are accomplished. The access and target dimensions have many interfaces and thus have a multitude of combinations that expand their range and power. These interfaces contribute to both the well-being and the stress of the family. As with Kantor and Lehr’s access and target dimensions, family eldercare includes both quantitative and qualitative components. Examples are the amount and type of eldercare actually provided (quantitative) and the meaning that caring for one’s own has for the individual members involved with that care (qualitative). The power that individuals exert in decision-making involving eldercare offers another qualitative example. While the present study cannot address qualitative aspects of eldercare to any great degree, the quantitative or physical aspects of caring offer much opportunity for analyses related to caregiving’s influence on employment adaptation. The influence of eldercare on employment adaptation is likely to vary due to the changing needs of the elderly person and altered family circumstances. Thus, the primary caregiver and the family make many decisions to address those issues. As in other events or developmental changes that occur during the family life cycle, the family system experiences periods of equilibrium and disequilibrium with eldercare. In time, most caregivers and their families adjust personal and work roles, but the eventual symmetry or equilibrium shifts with the care recipient’s next period of change. The adaptation process provides needed stabilization for the family and the caregiver with multiple employment and family obligations. Clearly, the norm of family care in society (an output of the decision-making process) involves many changing decisions, circumstances, and interactions over the life oourse. sustena rnemh< and er caregi halant cultur from with the it. those emp] Only Cmp Gide Inee mm Emp 33 course. Bubolz and Sontag (in press) state that "a family carries out physical-biological sustenance, economic maintenance, and psychosocial and nurturance functions for its members. " In order to manage family eldercare responsibilities, decisions about choices and exchanges permeate family relationships and interactions and influence the primary caregiver’s employment adaptation. Employment adaptation, stemming from caregiver decisions that attempt to balance work and family obligations, varies with the corporate work site culture. A culture sympathetic to caregiving concerns provides a very different work environment from one where family problems must be "kept at home” and not allowed to interfere with employment responsibilities. However, regardless of the workplace environment, the interactive work and caregiving situations cannot be separated. Ripple effects of those interactions permeate all levels: individual employees and their family members, employers and co—workers, and society. The employee’s adaptation to competing family and work roles may influence not only the quality of care provided to the elderly relative and relationships within the family as a whole, but also work quality, productivity, and the ability to remain employed. Likewise, the involvement of the caregiver and family assistance in meeting eldercare obligations may influence how the caregiver ultimately adapts employment to meet care demands. As ecological theory suggests, the consequences or outcomes of caregiver decisions involve multiple levels. In addition to individual, family, and employer influences, society ultimately suffers from an extreme employment adaptation, the employee decision to quit work. The repercussions and ramifications, although not immediate purchasing of public employed possible c Bl 0f multi¥ present st influence the role t] 0f family flamewo] the Social f“Inland ”Scythe 34 immediately apparent, may be declines in the gross national product (decreased purchasing power), foregone income tax revenue (decreased income), and increased use of public assistance programs (inadequate income to meet nwds). As the number of employed family caregivers increases because of projected demographic changes, these possible outcomes gain significance for society. Bubolz and Sontag (in press) maintain that the ecoperspective allows examination of multi-level functions and systems in relation to each other and over time. The present study acknowledges the dynamic nature of caregiving and seeks to determine the influence that eldercare responsibility has on employment adaptation from inception of the role through a three-month period. The ecoperspective permits the interactive nature of family processes and relationships to be captured and studied. Also, the ecological framework provides a more comprehensive approach to analyzing human behavior than the social systems model (Bubolz, Eicher, & Sontag, 1979). The social systems model frequently minimizes interrelationships between natural and social systems. This researcher recognizes the strong interaction between work and family. Ex h Th 0 The choice and exchange theory is versatile in that its concepts can be applied to a wide variety of situations ranging from one-to-one encounters to group interactions (Nye, 1979; Rook, 1987). In this study, the caregiver and employment adaptation represented individual choices and exchanges, while the caregiver assistance from the family denoted group interaction and involvement related to eldercare. The exchanges may be specific, as when limited to a few individuals, or very diverse, as those that llll’Oll provir relatit qualit work PICPC u'me. child perio influc l0 mg 35 involve individuals and society (Nye, 1979). Rook (1987) suggests that the theory provides the potential to analyze not only quantitative, but qualitative aspects of relationships. The present study offers a quantitative approach, but also includes qualitative potential by the examination of open-ended responses about other effects on work that may occur due to eldercare responsibility. Exchanges with parents and children vary over the life course. From a preponderance of parental ”giving" to very young children, the pendulum shifts over time. At the end of the parental life cycle, the primary giving may switch to the adult child as the eldercare role evolves. These " giving" behaviors and activities are usually performed for positive reasons; however, a negative perspective (or least cost) may also influence the interaction. For example, adult children may provide parent care in order to meet societal norms and avoid censure from others. Eldercare by an employed caregiver involves decisions that influence not only the care situation, but also the workplace. When the employee chooses to arrive late, leave early, or be absent for extended periods, the decision impacts other employees left to do the job. Co-workers who pick up the slack may resent that necessity, even when they are aware of the reason. When employees elect to reduce hours or quit work, they gain more time to provide care, but sacrifice employment benefits, the opportunity for advancement, and the socialization and satisfaction employment can provide. Employment and other roles (unless carried to an extreme) may enhance caregiver well-being by buffering the stresses stemming from the care situation, linking external resources, and increasing caregiver feelings of self-confidence and worth (Stoller & Pugliesi, 1989a). Quitting work may increase the caregiver’s problems. Further, when family have g; in orde elderca (‘0um have 3 Chang. elderc; mal’ be Dim-id. deClSlC 36 caregivers reduce work hours, take extended leaves, or quit employment in order to provide care, management faces recruitment and training costs for new workers hired to fill the void. Summary During the past six years, studies began to address the competing demands of family eldercare and employment. Researchers, through cross-sectional investigations, have garnered impressive data about the employment adaptation many caregivers make in order to meet their family obligations. While some employers are oblivious to the eldercare impact on their employees and the resulting influence that it may have on the company’s bottom line, that trend is changing. Companies that take a proactive approach have assessed their work force and altered policies and procedures to address the changing needs of employees with eldercare responsibilities. While the impact of the eldercare phenomenon is less clear for society than for the workplace, its ramifications may be projected. The use of the human ecological and the choice and exchange theories provides a versatile framework to study eldercare and the workplace and related decisions, interactions, and adaptation over time. mEnt. land [ CHAPTER III Methods The study explored the influence of acquiring the family eldercare role on the employment adaptation of women. The data utilized were from a longitudinal caregiver study at Michigan State University, ”Caregiver Responses to Managing Elderly Patients at Home” (NIA #2 R01 AGO6584), referred to in this paper as the core study. This chapter includes discussion of the conceptual model, the research design, objectives, questions, hypotheses, and variables for the study. The instrumentation, sampling procedure (core study and this research), data collection techniques, and the methods of data analysis used conclude this section. Figure 1 displays a preliminary conceptual model for family eldercare and its influence on employment adaptation over a three-month time period. In this model, selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement influence the employment adaptations that female family caregivers make. These employment adaptations, in turn, influence family support and caregiver involve- ment. While family caregiving and decisions made related to eldercare influence society (and the reverse), that effect is not included in this model. 37 ‘- a r-- — v-‘fJ‘ 38 30:833.. EoE>oEEm uca Sacco—em 2:5“. .3. .235. 3:33:60 EaSE=oE .p 2:2“. :. __ _ mcozmamu< EmE>oEEm :oaazm 2E8 E __ _ wcozmamg EmESEEw a 9:5 2:85 8:: : mg: 8385 O D EmEm>_o>c_ 520050 82256920 Emfiamm Emu ..mZmeQ v EmEm>_o>c_ $2960 all! (Cr in he pt 39 The study involved three blocks of independent variables. The first, caregiver and care recipient characteristics, included caregiver age, relationship to the care recipient, marital status, education, occupation, household income, employment status (current full-time or part-time) co-residence status (now and before caregiving), and the care recipient gender and age. The second, family support, encompassed the size of the family network and the amount and frequency of family assistance provided to either the caregiver or the care recipient. Caregiver involvement, the third block, included the total help provided with activities of daily living (ADL), instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), and health care activities (HCA), and also the time demands (in hours) of physical care and supervision of the elderly relative. Three types of employment adaptation comprised the dependent variables. Type I employment adaptation was defined as maintaining, but altering one’s work role. Possible modifications included: (a) arriving late/leaving early, (b) missing work without pay, (c) taking sick/personal days, (d) changing work hours, (e) refusing jobs or promotions, (f) not looking for jobs or better jobs, and (g) other ways that caregiving was affecting work. Type II adaptation was interpreted as taking a leave of absence, and Type III adaptation meant quitting or retiring from employment in order to provide care. Figure 2 displays the independent and dependent variables for the study. 40 333:5 .coaconoa use Essence—E. .« 2:2“. co__m.amu< z. 3: co__m_amo< _ :_ mat; _ lllllllr. __ we: co._m_amo< _ mg; 3998,“. emoumo a 9:6 8.8.2 85.» O EzomEo n 00 1 I _ 2 9:5 8.68:." r. 1 8.2285 .0 9:0: .cmEm>_0>c_ Ezaemo _l 9.8 .mann. .o everl— _cme>_0>c_ 00 .93 _ QEI 2.83 5:03de _ . Baa: _l 99.. 3.8mm anE< 1— l _>=Emmm _l {0362 2.83 L j 880 5 L 1 m2 mo L 858m w .5on 8:85me .00 Cat”: 3.9m EmESEEw 00 088: l 8.6.6.0920 _l _ 00 ll— .cmafiom mao 1 8.8380 8 L . $28.8 —l c2838 8 l_ _l 3292.5: 8 l— _l 958.66,. 00 l— 7 mo< 8 L l Hagga— rol of r.) Re 41 Research Obj ectives The overall purpose of the study was to investigate the influence of acquiring the role of caregiver for a physically disabled elderly relative on the employment adaptation of women. Unless specified otherwise, the following specific objectives encompassed each type of adaptation. 1. To determine the immediate and delayed (after three months) employment adaptation for the female employee. To determine the influence of selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics (caregiver age, relationship to care recipient, marital status, education, occupation, household income, employment status, co-residence status, care recipient age and gender) on caregiver employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. To determine the influence of family support (number in network, amount of help, and frequency of family assistance with eldercare) on caregiver employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. To determine the influence of caregiver involvement (assistance with ADL, IADL, and HCA and the hours of both physical care and supervision) on employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. To assess the probability that the caregiver displays Type II or Type III adaptation at Time 2 given the level of Type 1 adaptation at Time 1. Research Design This research, using a longitudinal design (panel study), examined how selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement inf’. Shut the 1198 oft and ma} holi rela- melt llll-‘o COnc‘ in M 42 influenced employment adaptation. Currently, cross-sectional designs predominate in the study of caregiving’s influence on employment. This makes it impossible to represent the varied decisions that caregivers make over time in response to changing care recipient needs. Longitudinal studies provide a mechanism to determine critical periods when a caregiver adapts employment in order to provide care. Of course, the reverse, to stop caregiving in order to maintain employment, also may result. The longitudinal approach of the present study enhanced the understanding of the dynamics and interaction of work and family care. Care recipient, caregiver, and family needs change and result in adaptation by the caregiver in order to meet the new care requirements. Employment responsibilities also may shift, thus creating the environment for additional adaptation by the employed family caregiver. The choice of a longitudinal design considered the ecological concept of adaptation and acknowledged that caregiving is a dynamic, not a static process. The study encompassed two waves of data collection; inception of the eldercare role (Time 1) and at three months (Time 2). Human ecology theory mandates that humans and their environments be viewed holistically. While it is impossible for any research to examine all possible variables related to a situation, a study can deal with multiple issues. In the present study, these include selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics, family support, caregiver involvement, and possible employment adaptations. The study was non-experimental and conducted in the natural setting. Female employed family caregivers (N =236), living in Michigan, comprised the units studied. Res prir 43 Research Questions and Hypotheses All research questions and hypotheses referred to the employed female who had primary family caregiving responsibility for a physically disabled elderly relative. 1. Does employment adaptation occur, and if so, does the adaptation type differ between Time 1 (inception) and Time 2 (three months)? a. What Type 1 adaptation (maintain, but alter work) occurs? arrive late/leave early miss work without pay take sick/personal days change work hours refuse job/promotion kept from job hunt/better job other work effects b. What Type II adaptation (leave of absence) occurs? c. What Type III adaptation (quit/retire) occurs? Hla: A variety of Type I employment adaptations occur at both Time 1 and Time 2. Hlb: There are fewer Type I adaptive behaviors at Time 1 than at Time 2. ch: There is less Type II adaptation at Time 1 than at Time 2. Hld: There is no difference between Type III adaptation at Time 1 and Type III adaptation at Time 2. 2. 44 What is the probability that Time 1, Type I employment adaptation (maintain, but alter work) influences Time 2, Type 11 (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/retire) employment adaptations? H2a: The use of Time 1, Type I adaptation influences the use of Time 2, Type II and Type III adaptations. What is the relationship of selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics to each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? caregiver age relationship to the care recipient caregiver marital status caregiver education caregiver occupation caregiver household income employment status (current part-time or full-time) co-residence status (now and before) care recipient age care recipient gender '-‘"-"':-="!° fine? 9'? H3a: Caregiver age relates to each type of employment adaptation. H3b: Employment adaptation differs for spouse and daughter/daughter-in-law caregivers. H3c: Employment adaptation differs by relationship to care recipient. H3d: Caregiver education relates to employment adaptation. H3c: Employment adaptation differs with caregiver occupation. H3f: Caregiver household income relates to employment adaptation. H3g: Employment adaptation differs with full—time and part-time employment status. 45 H3h: Employment adaptation differs if caregiver and care recipient reside together. H3i: Care recipient age relates to employment adaptation. H3j: Care recipient gender makes no difference in employment adaptation. How much variation in each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2 do selected significant caregiver and care recipient characteristics explain? a caregiver age b relationship to the care recipient c caregiver education (1. caregiver household income e co-residence status (now) f care recipient gender H4a: Caregiver age explains more variation in employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2 than do other caregiver/care recipient characteristics. H4b: Care recipient age and care recipient gender explain little variation in employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. Was there a reported change in the amount of help from family members between Time 1 and Time 2? H5a: The majority of caregivers will report that the level of family help remained "about the same" at Time 2. 6. 46 What is the relationship of family support to each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? a. number in network b. amount of help provided c. frequency of assistance given H6a: The number in the family network has no relationship to employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. H6b: The amount of help provided by the family relates to employment adaptation at both Time 1 and Time 2. H6c: The frequency of family help relates to employment adaptation at both Time 1 and Time 2. How much variation in each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2 does family support explain? a. number in network b. amount of help provided c. frequency of assistance given H7a: The amount of help and frequency of assistance provided explains more variation in employment adaptation at both Time 1 and Time 2 than does the number in the network. Do caregiving responsibilities disrupt normal work and daily activities at both time periods? H8a Caregiving responsibilities disrupt normal work and daily activities at both time periods. 9. 10. ll. 47 Does the level of reported caregiver involvement change from Time 1 to Time 2? H9a: The majority of caregivers will report that they provide ”about the same" amount of care compared to three months ago. What is the relationship of caregiver involvement (activities and time) to each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? a. total involvement (ADL, IADL, and HCA) b. hours of physical care c. hours of supervision H10a: Total involvement (ADL, IADL, and HCA) relates to employment adaptation. H10b: Hours of physical care relate to employment adaptation. H10c: Hours of supervision relate to employment adaptation. How much variation in each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2 is explained by caregiver involvement (activities and time)? a. total involvement (ADL, IADL, and HCA) b. hours of physical care c. hours of supervision H1 la: Hours of physical care explain more variation in employment adaptation than total involvement or hours of supervision at both Time 1 and Time 2. 48 12. Which identified key variables from questions 4, 7, and 11 are the best predictors of each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? (TypeI = caregiver age, relationship to care recipient, caregiver gender, amount of family help, hours of physical care; Type II = caregiver education, co- residence now, amount of family help, hours of physical care; Type 111 = caregiver household income, family network, frequency of family help, total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care) H12a: The hours of physical care are the best predictors of each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. H12b: The amount of family help at both Time 1 and Time 2 is less effective in predicting employment adaptation than are the other variables in the equation. H12c: Caregiver and care recipient characteristics explain more variation in employment adaptation than does caregiver involvement. Research Variables This section contains the conceptual and operational definitions of the dependent and independent variables. Unless otherwise stated, the definitions applied to both Time 1 and Time 2. Questions for Time 1 alluded to the previous month, while Time 2 questions referred to the prior three months. The portions of the instrument used for both the study and the operational definitions are in Appendix A. In addition to specific work adaptations noted at both Time 1 and Time 2, caregivers were asked at Time 2 if their employment status had changed in the past three months and if so, how it had changed the type Depender EWID Arrive L; hours as ; 01' leave e bliss “’0!- Co to DTOVide Work Witht Take Siclg; COn PETSOnal da; ope: days or take no). At both 49 changed (open-ended response). The percentage of those indicating a change and also the type of alteration was determined by their responses. Winks Type I Employment Adaptation (maintain, but alter work) Arrive Late/ Leave Early Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver worked the entire shift or worked hours as assigned. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated if caregiving caused her to be late or leave early (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). Miss Work without Pay Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver took time off without pay in order to provide care. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated if caregiving caused her to miss work without pay (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). Take Sick/Personal Days Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver used either paid sick time or personal days off in order to provide care. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated if caregiving caused her to use sick days or take personal days in order to provide care (yes or no response; 1 = yes; 2 = no). At both Time 1 and Time 2, caregivers indicated the specific number of work days lost (withir numbers, I Change W Co could mar Op change he RBfUSC Jot Co PrOmotiOn Kept frOm Cor.- liCl' job l: looking for 50 lost (within the past three months) because of caregiving responsibilities. From these numbers, the mean and standard deviation for each time period was determined. Change Work Hours Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver changed her work schedule so she could manage her caregiving tasks. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether caregiving caused her to change her work hours (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). Refuse Job or Promotion Conceptual definition: Whether or not the caregiver refused a job offer or a promotion so that she could manage her caregiving tasks. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether caregiving caused her to turn down a job or promotion (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). Kept from Job Hunt/Better Job Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver was unable to look for a job or a better job because of caregiving responsibility. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether caregiving kept her from looking for a job or a better job (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). 5 1 Other Work Effects Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver experienced any additional, ”other" effects related to work not specifically defined as Type I adaptations. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether caregiving affected her work in ways not mentioned (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). If the response was yes, the caregiver described how caregiving affected her work. The results of this item and the preceding work alteration items were summed to create an index of Type I employment adaptation (range from 0-7). The index of Type I employment adaptation was then used to obtain a mean score for the sample. This provided a summary measure of the Type I employment adaptation that caregivers experienced. A high score indicated more caregiver Type I adaptation to employment than did a low score. The open-ended responses generated with "other work effects" were grouped according to patterns of similarity and provided a qualitative supplement to the quantitative information about employment adaptation. Groupings from Time 1 data included: 2 = no, 3 = emotional stress, 4 = cannot do other things, 5 = cannot concentrate, 6 = schedule, and 7 = other. Response patterns for Time 2 varied slightly and comprised: 2 = no, 3 = more tired, 4 = more stressed, 5 = worry causes lack of concentration, 6 = quality of work suffers, 7 = turned down better job, 8 = loss of benefits, and 9 = other ("1 =" was not used for either Time 1 or Time 2). The percentage of caregivers who responded yes to ”other work effects" was subdivided into these descriptive categories. At both Time 1 and Time 2, caregivers were asked to indicate how disruptive caregiving was to their work and daily activities. The response choices included: 52 1 = not at all, 2 = a little, and 3 = somewhat, and 4 = a great deal. The percentage of caregivers who responded to each choice was determined. Type II Employment Adaptation (leave of absence) Conceptual definition: Whether or not the caregiver took extended time off from work to provide care. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether caregiving caused her to take a leave of absence from her job (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). The percentage of caregivers who answered yes was obtained from the responses. Yype III Employment Adaptation (quit/reti re) Conceptual definition: Whether the caregiver quit or retired from her job so that she could provide care. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether caregiving had caused her to quit or retire from her job (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). The percentage of caregivers who answered yes was obtained from the responses. Wines Caregiver Relationship to Care Recipient Conceptual definition: How the caregiver was related to the care recipient. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated her relationship to the recipient. Choices in the original questionnaire included: 1 = spouse, 2 = parent, 3 = daughter, 4 = son, 5 = daughter-in-law, 6 = son-in-law, 7 = sister, 8 = brother, recode Careg Care é [hep C0 rel Chm Sch0 Fibs, 53 9 = sister-in-law, 10 = brother-in—law, 11 = granddaughter, 12 = grandson, 13 = niece, 14 = nephew, 15 = aunt, 16 = uncle, 17 = other relative, 18 = friend/companion, or 19 = other non-relative to the care recipient. These were recoded for the present study so that 1 = spouse, 2 = daughter/daughter—in-law, and 3 = other. Caregiver Age Conceptual definition: The number of years the caregiver had been alive. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated the date of her birth. Caregiver Marital Status Conceptual definition: The caregiver’s current marital status. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether she was: 1 = single, 2 = married, 3 = divorced, 4 = widowed, or 5 = separated. These were recoded for the present study so that 1 = married and 2 = unmarried. Caregiver Education Conceptual definition: The number of years that the caregiver attended school. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated the level of education attained. Choices in the original questionnaire included: 1 = grade school or less, 2 = some high school, 3 = completed high school, 4 = some college, 5 = completed college, 6 = some graduate school, or 7 = completed graduate school. These were recoded for the present study so that 1 = some high school or less, 2 = high school, 3 = some college, 54 and 4 == college or more for descriptive purposes. Education was also recoded as a continuous variable (6 = completed grade school or less, 9 = completed some high school, 12 = completed high school, 14 = completed some college/technical school, 16 = completed college, 18 = completed some graduate school, 20 = completed graduate/professional degree) for analytical reasons (correlations and multivariate analyses). Caregiver Occupation Conceptual definition: The caregiver’s type of employment. Conceptual definition: For the original questionnaire, the caregiver indicated her occupation (an open-ended response). The responses were then coded: l = professional, management, administrative; 2 = sales, clerical, technical; 3 = craft or operative; 4 = laborer; 5 = farmer; 6 = homemaker; 7 = other. These were recoded for the present study so that l = professional, management, administrative; 2 = sales, clerical, technical; and 3 = other. Caregiver Household Income Conceptual definition: The household income of the caregiver. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated the appropriate gross income (before taxes) for her household: 1 = 0-1999; 2 = 2000-3999; 3 = 4000-5999; 4 = 6000-7999; 5 = 8000-9999; 6 = 10000-13,999; 7 = 14,000-17,999; 8= 18,000-2l,999; 9 = 22,000-25,999; 10 = 26,000-29,999; 11 = 30,000-34,999; 12 = 35,000-39,999; 13 = 40,000-44,999; 14 = 45,000-49,999; 15 = 50,000—54,999; 16 = 55,000-59,999; 55 and 17 = 60,000 and over. The present study used the same coding to allow a more realistic report of the mean income than recoding into smaller groupings would allow. Enployment Status (prior to caregiving) Conceptual definition: The amount of paid work the caregiver was doing prior to caregiving responsibility. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether she was employed outside the home prior to caregiving (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). If the response was yes, the caregiver next indicated whether the employment was full-time or part-time (l = full-time, 2 = part-time). Caregivers who responded yes (employed prior to caregiving) comprised the sample (N =236) for the present study. Current Employment Status Conceptual definition: The current employment status of the caregiver. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether she was currently employed full-time, part-time, self-employed, retired, leave of absence, unemployed, not employed for pay, or quit work to care. Choices were: 1 = full-time, 2 = part-time, 3 = self-employed, 4 = retired, 5 = leave of absence, 6 = unemployed, 7 = not employed for pay, and 8 = quit work to care. The present study used 1 = full-time (includes self-employed), 2 = part-time, 3 = leave of absence, 4 = quit/retire, and 5 = unemployed. The instrument for Time 2 did not include the question about current status. Instead, the caregiver was asked if there had been a change in employment status in the last three months, and if so, how it shifted. Response choices included 1 = full-time, different j Co-Reside Co living quar op currently I; 2 = no). We recipi. 2 = no). Care Recipz' Conc Oper Unable t0 re: 56 full-time, 2 = part-time, 3 = quit working, 4 = leave of absence, 5 = retired, 6 = different job/changed jobs, and 7 = work more as a caregiver. Co-Residence with Care Recipient Conceptual definition: Whether or not the caregiver and the care recipient shared living quarters. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated whether the care recipient currently lived in the same household with the caregiver (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). If the response was yes, the caregiver was asked whether she lived with the care recipient prior to assuming the caregiver role (yes or no response; 1 = yes, 2 = no). Care Recipient Age Conceptual definition: The number of years that the care recipient had been alive. Operational definition: The care recipient (or caregiver if the care recipient was unable to respond) indicated the date of the care recipient’s birth. Care Recipient Gender Conceptual definition: Whether the care recipient was male or female. Operational definition: The care recipient (or caregiver if the care recipient was unable to respond) indicated the care recipient’s gender to the interviewer ( 1 = male, 2 = female). Fami. mern the r fami 1989 men Fan reel 57 Family Network Conceptual definition: The number in the caregiver’s family. Operational definition: The caregiver identified her living adult relatives and how far (in miles) they lived from the caregiver. From that figure, the number of network members who lived 50 miles or less from the caregiver was determined. The mean of the network number living within 50 miles of the caregiver provided the measure of family members available to help with eldercare. The Given studies (Given & Given, 1989) of the core data used the 50 mile criterion as a basis for determining those network members most likely to offer aid. The same concept was used in the present study. Family Help (amount) Conceptual definition: The amount of help with eldercare that the caregiver received from her family network. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated the amount of help that each adult relative provided with caregiving (1 = none or very little, 2 = a little, 3 = some, 4 = quite a bit, 5 = a great deal). The mean score for the amount of help given by network members who lived within 50 miles of the caregiver provided the measure of family help with eldercare. At Time 2, the caregiver indicated whether in the past three months, overall help from the family increased (I), stayed the same (2), or decreased (3). Family Assistance frequency) Conceptual definition: The caregiver’s recognition of the frequency of family help with caregiving responsibilities. provi ofth item: Care healr felat dail} 38th wall 58 Operational definition: The caregiver indicated how often anyone in the family provided assistance in the following ways: helped with physical care spent time keeping the care recipient company stayed with the care recipient so that the caregiver could do something else for a few hours gave the caregiver emotional support helped with transportation (caregiver or recipient) helped with money or other material goods checked on the caregiver to see if she was alright provided or encouraged diversional activities for the care recipient 99‘!” FPS“??- Response choices for each item were: 1 = rarely or none of the time, 2 = some of the time, 3 = most of the time, 4 = almost all of the time. The mean of the summed items provided the measure of family assistance given. Caregiver Total Involvement Conceptual definition: The help with physical (ADL), instrumental (IADL), and health care activities (HCA) of daily living that the caregiver provided for her elderly relative. Operational definition: The caregiver described her involvement with activities of daily living (ADL), instrumental activities of daily living (IADL), and health care activities (HCA) of daily living. The ADL categories included dressing, eating, bathing, walking inside the house, toileting, and transferring in and out of bed. The possible responses to how frequently the caregiver helped the care recipient were: 0 = never, 1 = once a week or less, 2 = several times a week, 3 = once a day, 4 = several times daily. The sum of the individual item scores in the ADL section represented the ADL involver than dic l housewr section, ranged scores i more c; OXFgen ids, [u Ulcers/1 medica Katha by are 59 involvement score. High scores indicated more caregiver involvement with ADL tasks than did low scores. Caregiver involvement with IADL activities included cooking/preparing meals, housework, shopping, laundry, transportation, and money management. As in the ADL section, the possible responses to how frequently the caregiver helped the care recipient ranged from 0 = never to 4 = several times daily. The sum of the individual item scores in the IADL section represented the IADL score. Here, too, high scores indicated more caregiver involvement with IADL duties than did low scores. Caregiver involvement with HCA functions (urinary catheter/catheter care, oxygen administration, IV, Hickman, Broviac catheter care/dressing, IV medications/flu- ids, tube or IV feedings, injections, special exercises/physical therapy, care of ulcers/bedsores, slo'n care, colostomy care, care of post-operative incision, oral medications, nasogastric tube and care, incontinence of urine, incontinence of stool, tracheostomy/tracheostomy care, respirator/care of respirator, suctioning) was measured by asking the caregiver if the care recipient required any of the activities. For the indicated activities, the frequency of assistance provided by the caregiver was assessed. Scores for each indicated item were: 0 = never, 1 = once a week or less, 2 = several times a week, 3 = once a day, 4 = several times daily. The sum of the scores for pertinent items in the HCA section provided the health care activity involvement score. As with ADL and IADL scores, the higher the score, the greater the caregiver involvement in those activities. The sum of the scores from ADL, IADL, and HCA sections represented the caregiver’s total involvement with those activities. Although the total involvement score was the measure, the sum of each section enabled the d1 1 and montl Hours theca Ueatm. PTOVid; Hours . hours). Safest) 1 number 60 the determination of internal fluctuation with ADL, IADL, and HCA functions at Time 1 and Time 2. At Time 2, the caregiver indicated the amount of care compared to three months ago. Response choices were: 1 = less care, 2 = about the same, and 3 = more eare. Hours of Physical Care Conceptual definition: The time the caregiver spent providing physical care for the care recipient. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated, in a usual day, how many hours (0-24) were spent providing physical assistance (activities such as eating, dressing, treatments, etc.) for her relative. A mean score for the number of hours per day spent providing care provided the measure. Hours of Supervision Conceptual definition: The time the caregiver spent supervising the care recipient. Operational definition: The caregiver indicated, in a usual day (excluding night hours), how many hours were spent supervising (being with the recipient to ensure safety) her relative. Hours could range from 0 to 20 per day. A mean score for the number of hours per day spent supervising care provided the measure. Instrumentation Telephone interviews and self-report questionnaires provided the data for the core study, "Caregiver Responses to Managing Elderly Patients at Home. " The portions of instrur core 3 conten sound 61 the instrument to be used in the present research included the following sections from the telfiphofle interview: (3) W1. 0)) W. (C) mm. (d) We: (of the family). and (e) CaregixcLlnxoLvernent and (O W. The research team of Given, Given, Collins, King, and Stommel developed the instruments (with the exception of the ADL and IADL items taken from OARS) for the core study. Experts and peers evaluated the measures and concluded they had face, content, and construct validity. All tools used in the core study are psychometrically sound research instruments that have been tested on over 600 caregivers during the first caregiver study, "Caregiver’s Response to Managing Elderly Patients at Home” (Caregiver I). Examples from the instruments are in Appendix A. The dependent variables, employment adaptations (Type I, Type II, and Type III), were described at two intervals; one at inception of the caregiver role (Time 1) and the second at three months (Time 2). The measure used was the W section of the core instrument. This survey assessed how many times in the last month (or three months for Time 2) the employee adapted employment in order to meet caregiving responsibilities. Employment adaptation, with the exception of "other work effects” (Type I), which required an open-ended response, was self-reported as yes or no (1 = yes; 2 = no) and included: a. Type I employment adaptation (maintain, but alter work) arrive late/leave early miss work without pay change work hours take sick/personal days refuse job or promotion ker b. T): c. Ty] At b0tl 62 kept from job hunt/better job other work effects b. Type II employment adaptation (leave of absence) c. Type III employment adaptation (quit/retire) At both Time 1 and Time 2, the caregiver indicated how many work days were missed in order to provide care (open-ended response) and also how disruptive caregiving had been on normal activities. Response choices for disruption of activities included: 1 = not at all 2 = a little 3 = somewhat 4 = a great deal The screening instrument and the Time 1 telephone questionnaire for the core study contributed the caregiver and care recipient sociodemographic information for the present study. The data included: 1. 2. Care recipient gender (1 = male; 2 = female). Caregiver gender (1 = male; 2 = female). Care recipient birth date _month, _day, _year. Caregiver birth date _month, _day, _year. Caregiver marital status (1 = single, never married, 2 = married/remarried, 3 = divorced, 4 = widowed, 5 = separated). This was recoded for the present study (1 = married, 2 = unmarried). Caregiver relationship to care recipient (l = spouse, 2 = parent, 3/4 = daughter/son, 5/6 = daughter/son-in-law, 7/ 8 = sister/brother, 9/10 = sister/bro- ther-in-law, 11/12 = granddaughter/son, 13/ 14 = niece/nephew, 15/ 16 = aunt/uncle, 17 = other relative, 18 = friend/companion, 19 = other non-relative). This was recoded for the present study (1 = spouse, 2 = daughter/daughter-in- law, 3 = other). Caregiver highest level of education (1 = completed grade school or less, 2 = completed some high school, 3 = completed high school, 4 = completed some college/technical training, 5 = completed college, 6 = completed some graduate/professional school, 7 = completed graduate/professional degree). This was r or 1e colic, conti prese scho scho corn] 3. Care cleri horn man 9. Live YES, 10. H01 5,9< 17' 30,1 15 family. ' their diSt 10. 63 was recoded for the present study for descriptive purposes (1 = some high school or less, 2 = high school, 3 = some college/technical training, 4 = completed college/technical training or more). The core study values were also recoded to continuous variables for correlations and multivariate analyses performed in the present study (6 = completed grade school or less, 9 = completed some high school, 12 = completed high school, 14 = completed some college/technical school, 16 = completed college, 18 = completed some graduate school, 20 = completed graduate/professional degree. Caregiver occupation -(l = professional, management, administrative; 2 = sales, clerical, technical, 3 = craft or operative, 4 = laborer; 5 = merchant; 6 = homemaker; 7 = other. This was recoded for the present study (1 = professional, management, administrative; 2 = sales, clerical, technical; 3 = other). Live in the same household, now (1 = yes, 2 = no); if the response was yes, the caregiver was asked about co-residence before assuming the caregiver role (1 = yes, 2 = no). Household gross income before taxes (1 = 0-1,999; 2 = 2,000-3,999; 3 = 4,000- 5,999; 4 = 6,000-7,999; 5 = 8,000-9,999; 6 = 10,000-13,999; 7 = 14,000- 17,999; 8 = l8,000-21,999; 9 = 22,000-25,9999; 10 = 26,000-29,999; 11 = 30,000-34,999; 12 = 35,000-39,999; 13 = 40,000-44,999; 14 = 45,000-49,999; 15 = 50,000-54,999; 16 = 55,000-59,999; 17 = 60,000 and over). The Family Network instrument assessed information about the caregiver’s family. The caregiver listed family members (spouses, children, siblings, and in-laws), their distance (in miles) from the caregiver, and then indicated how much help with eldercare that each network member provided. Response choices were: 1 = none or very little 2 = a little 3 = some 4 = quite a bit 5 = a great deal At Time 2, the caregiver indicated whether overall help from the family increased, decreased, or stayed ”about the same" during the past three months. The la caregivers pe The present Reliability fo roar-”9999:.” :- (D Ath-d II H H H s? (Time 1) am “8 used to items that l-Z Mimics of Mimics 0f dres: eatin bathr Walk lone. 64 The last instrument used for the study, the W scale, appraised the caregiver’s perception of the frequency of assistance received from family and friends. The present study used only the portion dealing with the family of the caregiver. Reliability for the W scale was alpha .69. Items included: helped with physical care spent time keeping your _ company stayed with _ so that you could do something else for a few hours given you emotional support or encouragement helped with transportation; for either you or care recipient helped you with money or other material goods checked on yoo to be sure that you were all right provided or encouraged diversional activities for (care recipient) such as cards or Scrabble, etc. 5"?" rm 99 9‘9 Caregiver response possibilities were: rarely or none of the time some of the time most of the time almost all of the time ammu— II II II II The independent variable, caregiver involvement, was measured at inception (Time 1) and at three months (Time 2). The Involvement portion of the core instrument was used to determine the caregiver’s responsibility for eldercare. This section contained items that tapped the degree of assistance with physical, instrumental, and health care activities of daily living that the caregiver provided for the care recipient. Physical activities of daily living (ADL) included: dressing eating bathing walking inside the house toileting transferring lnstrun Possib? activiti The he Possibr. 65 Instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) included: cooking housework shopping laundry transportation money management Possible responses for how often the caregiver provided help with both ADL and IADL activities were: 0 = never 1 = once week/less several times/week once a day 2 3 4 several trmes/ day The health care activities (HCA) included: urinary catheter/catheter care oxygen administration IV/Hickman or Broviac catheter care/dressing IV medications/fluids tube or IV feedings injections (pain meds or insulin) special exercises/physical therapy care of ulcers/bedsores skin care (special cleansing lotions) colostomy/colostomy care care of post-operative wound/incision oral medications nasogastric tube and care incontinence of urine incontinence of stool tracheostomy/tracheostomy care respirator/ care of respirator suctioning Possible responses for how often the caregiver provided help with HCA were: never once week/less 0 1 2 several times/ week C s: 3 4 Relia‘. activities of impaired pa1 An : mu mregiver v number of bathing, d: night hour Physical a: per day; fr “Ether s °°mpared Sampling Tl, medical c' eldercare tion beCaL (dream! 66 3 = once a day 4 = several times/day Reliability for the Involvement scale was alpha 0.86 for caregiver involvement in activities of daily living, 0.82 for instrumental activities of daily living (for physically impaired patients), 0.52 for health care activities (based on 11 HCA). An additional assessment of caregiver involvement was obtained through Ibo W. The hours of both physical care and supervision spent by the caregiver were measured at inception and three months. The caregiver estimated the number of hours per day spent providing physical assistance (activities such as eating, bathing, dressing, treatments) and also calculated the number of hours per day (excluding night hours) consumed supervising the care recipient. Supervision time did not include physical assistance. Response possibilities for physical care ranged from 0 to 24 hours per day; for supervision, from 0 to 20 hours per day. At Time 2, the caregiver indicated whether she was providing more care, less care, or about the same amount of care compared to three months ago. Sampling Procedure The sample for the core study consisted of 630 family caregivers of elderly relatives. The majority of care recipients had a sudden, dramatic health crisis with a medical diagnosis involving the circulatory system. The recruitment criteria for the eldercare dyad follows. Patients (a) were aged 55 or older, (b) had a recent hospitaliza- tion because of some event or disease progression that qualified them for skilled home care, and (c) had increased dependencies (within the past 30 days) in at least one physical aetivity o (lADL) 0 member 1 had shift: The care in the st 1 criteria. elderca] (SD 67 activity of daily living (ADL) or at least two instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) or one equipment and/or health care activities. The caregiver was (a) the family member providing the most care and (b) during the 30 days prior to intake into the study had shifted assistance with one or more IADL activities to one or more ADL activities. The caregiving dyads were recruited by 27 sites (hospitals and visiting nurse agencies) in the state of Michigan, including one in the upper peninsula. The sample for the present study (N =236) met the following additional caregiver criteria. The caregivers were (a) female and (b) employed prior to assuming the eldercare role. The care recipient criteria remained as described in the preceding paragraph. At Time 2, the number of subjects in the core study dropped to 312. The decreased sample size was due to a variety of reasons. In some cases, the primary caregiver changed or the original caregiver decided not to continue with the study; in other instances, the care recipient either no longer required care, was hospitalized or placed in a nursing home, and still other care recipients died. A sort and match by identification number of Time 1 and Time 2 subjects for the present study indicated the original sample fell to 119 over the three-month period. Most caregivers were married (75.4%); the mean household income was $35,812 (SD = $17,426). Caregivers included daughters or daughters-in-law (60.6%), wives (29.7%), and other relatives (9.7%). The caregivers averaged about 51 (50.67; range = 19.59 - 75.96; SD = 11.97) years of age, while the mean care recipient age was approximately 73 (73.36; range = 53.11 to 93.30; SD = 9.16) years. Most care recipients were female (57.2%). Figure 3 contains other caregiver characteristics. . - ii .. i--. i it iiHi \CT. #iAL. Z ..... 32.: mUU 27.2 Z _ 2.2: D :75... Z 323.331....9Sula \ 68 Emasmm 3 i EmESEEm iv mocmuamm mamo c: -O ”v fidgw mc:H c_m©cmco tag me; m 3.3.8.0830 325930 .n 952“. $me 952E 9 mac 6me mg 6c 22: c “.202 .86 .m com<_mo:m_o\mm_mm .. 559368205 _m Al 8283000 iv 90869.60 863800 .U cosmosnm coo .10; the equation is recomputed omitting the removed variable, and the process is repeated until no variables can be removed. Next, the independent variable not in the equation with the smallest probability of F is entered if this value is smaller than .05. All variables in the equation are again examined for removal. The process continues until no variables in the equation need removal and no variables oot in the equation meet eligibility for entry requirements (Norusis, 1990b). With each variable entered into the equation, all variables are partialed out so the effect of the latest can be known (Glass & Hopkins, 1984). In the final step, the variables not meeting entry requirements are listed with the beta for each noted i_f the variable was entered at that point in the regression. 82 The regression equation is: Y = a + le + bX2 + bX3 +...bX6 + e where Y = dependent variable, employment adaptation a = the intercept between employment adaptation and the caregiver and care recipient variables b = the increase or decrease in Y for a one-unit change in X X, = caregiver age X2 = relationship to the care recipient X3 = caregiver education X4 = caregiver household income X5 = co-residence status now X6 = care recipient gender e = residual error Lewis-Beck (1989) offers the following regression assumptions: 1) no specifica- tion error (the relationship is linear, no relevant independent variables have been excluded, no irrelevant independent variables have been included), 2) no measurement error, 3) assumptions met that concern the error term (zero mean, homoscedasticity, no autocorrelation, the independent variable is uncorrelated with the error term, normality of distribution), and 4) the absence of perfect multicollinearity (none of the independent variables is perfectly correlated with either another or a linear combination of others). For Type II employment adaptation, two-group discriminant functional analysis (Wilks’s method) was used. This procedure, closely related to multiple regression, is used to identify relationships when the dependent variable is dichotomous (Norusis, 1990c). Norusis (1990c) notes the two sets of coefficients (discriminant and regression) are always proportional with two-group discriminant analysis. A desired outcome of discriminant analysis, as in regression, is the identification of the independent variables most important for group separation (good ”predictor" variables) and which are extraneous. Discriminant analysis, not multiple regression, was appropriate for analyzing 83 Type 11 (leave of absence) adaptation, because only a dichotomous response (yes or no) was available. Discriminant analysis generates standardized and unstandardized discriminant function coefficients. The unstandardized units are the ”multipliers of the variables when they are expressed in the original units" (Norusis, 1990c, p. B-l4). Standardizing the units to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 (as with multiple regression) produces the standardized discriminant function characteristic (Norusis, 1990c). The interpretation of the coefficients is similar to multiple regression. The magnitude of the unstandardized coefficients do not provide a good index of their relative importance to the equation if the variables differ in their measuring units (Norusis, 1990c). However, as with the beta coefficients of multiple regression, the standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients show the relative importance (but not the actual influence) of the predictor variables (Craft, 1990). According to Klecka (1980), the larger the magnitude of the standardized canonical discriminant function coefficient (regardless of the sign), the greater the variable contribution to the equation. However, the relative importance of any given variable depends on the variables included in the equation. Also, Klecka (1980) cautions that if two variables are highly correlated, they share their contribution to the score (even if that joint contribution to the score is very important), resulting in smaller standardized coefficients than if only one of the variables is used. He warns that the coefficients of two variables might be large, but with opposite signs, thus partially canceling the contribution of one variable by the reverse contribution of the other variable. 84 Grosof and Sardy (1985) offer the following assumptions for discriminant analysis: 1) at least two groups, 2) at least two cases per group, 3) any number of discriminating variables, but must be less than the number of cases minus two, 4) continuous interval or ratio discriminating variables (some authors relax this assumption), 5) independent discriminating variables; no one is a linear combination of the others, nor are any pair or subset highly intercorrelated, 6) each group is drawn from a population whose distribution on the discriminating variables is multivariate normal, and 7) the covariance matrices (intercorrelations) for each group must be approximately equal. Grosof and Sardy (1985) claim that assumptions six and seven are fairly robust, but warn that if there are many borderline cases, the accuracy of classification predictions can be impaired. The linear discriminant equation is: D=B+BX1+BX2 where D = the dependent variable, Type II employment adaptation B = coefficients, estimated from the data X = values of the independent variables X1 = caregiver education X2 = co-residence status A stepwise approach (Wilks’s method), in which variables are entered based on the smallest lambda for that step, was used. The stepwise approach produces an "optimal set of discriminating variables" (Klecka, p. 53, 1980). At each step (twice the number of independent variables), the variable that results in the smallest Wilks’s lambda is picked for entry (Norusis, 1990c). If a discriminatory variable contributes significantly to the explanation, it is included in the discriminant function; otherwise, it is not (Grosof & Sardy, 1985). Wilks’s lambda assesses discriminating power and considers both 85 differences between groups and the homogeneity within groups. Grosof and Sardy (1985) claim the Wilks’s statistic is preferred, since it can be converted into chi-square distributions, its values can be tested for significance, and it can be easily interpreted. An independent t-test (two-tailed) was used to check the significance of mean differences in caregiver household income with Time 1, Type III adaptation. According to Norusis (1990b), a t-test allows determination of whether two population means are equal. Norusis ( 1990b) maintains that the observed significance level from the t-test is the probability that differences at least as large as the observed difference would have occurred if the means were really equal. In the present study, the equality of means for the household income of those caregivers who chose to quit or retire at Time 1 and those who did not was the item of interest. An independent t-test (two-tailed) also was used to check the significance of mean differences in caregiver education with Time 2, Type HI adaptation. In the present study, the equality of means for the educational level attained for caregivers who chose to quit or retire at Time 2 and those who did not was the item of interest. Question 5: Was there a reported change in the amount of help from family members between Time 1 and Time 2? Time 2 data from the answers for that query were summarized. Then, frequen- cies for each possible response category were determined in order to answer this research question. 86 Question 6: What is the relationship of family support to each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? Pearson correlations were used to display the association of family support (number in network, amount of help provided, and frequency of assistance given) to employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. As noted, correlations require continuous dependent and independent variables; thus, they were appropriate to show the relationship of Type I (maintain, but alter work) employment adaptation to family support variables. As in Question 3, Type 11 (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/retire) employment adaptations were recoded to dummy variables before correlations with the independent variables were determined. The interpretation and assumptions of correlations remain as stated. Question 7: How much variation in each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2 does family support explain? Unlike Question 4, significant bivariate relationships with employment adaptation did not determine the independent variables used for subsequent analyses to answer this research question. All three variables were used in the regression and discriminant equations because of the importance of family in the conceptual model for this study. As in Question 4, stepwise multiple regression analysis showed the amount of variance in the dependent variable, Type I (maintain, but alter work) employment adaptation, explained by the independent variable, family support (number in network, amount of help provided, and frequency of assistance given) at Time 1 and Time 2. The regression assumptions and interpretation remain as stated with Question 4. The regression equation is: a: II II X ['0 11 II II II II 87 Type I employment adaptation the intercept between employment adaptation and the family support variables the increase or decrease in Y for a one unit change in X number in network amount of help provided frequency of assistance given residual error Two-group discriminant functional analysis (Wilks’s method) was used to determine whether family support could differentiate those caregivers who used Type 11 (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/ retire) employment adaptations from those who did not. The formerly noted assumptions and interpretation for discriminant analysis (Question 4) prevail. The linear discriminant equation is: where D B X X1 X2 X3 Time D=B+BX1+BX2+BX3 the dependent variable, Type II employment adaptation coefficients, estimated from the data values of the independent variables number in network amount of help provided frequency of assistance given 2 data did not suggest the need for additional analyses with family characteristic variables and Type 1, Type II, or Type III adaptations. 88 Question 8: Do caregiving responsibilities disrupt normal work and daily activities at both time periods? Data from Time 1 and Time 2 responses to this query were summarized. Then, frequencies for each possible category were calculated in order to answer this research question. Question 9: Does the level of reported caregiver involvement change from Time 1 to Time 2? Frequencies for reported changes in caregiver involvement at Time 2 were determined in order to answer this research question. Question 10: What is the relationship of caregiver involvement (activities and time) to each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? Pearson correlations were used to show the association of caregiver involvement (total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care, hours of supervision) to Type I (maintain, but alter work) employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. As noted, correlations require continuous dependent and independent variables; thus, they were appropriate to show the relationship of Type I employment adaptation to caregiver involvement variables. As in Questions 3 and 6, Type II and Type III employment adaptations were recoded to dummy variables before correlating with the caregiver involvement variables. The interpretation and assumptions of correlations remain as stated. 89 Question 11: How much variation in each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2 is explained by caregiver involvement (activities and time)? As in Question 4, bivariate associations among the variables and each adaptation type were examined to eliminate from multivariate analyses potential explanatory factors that did not have significant associations. Next, stepwise multiple regression analysis was used to determine the amount of variation in the dependent variable, Type I (maintain, but alter work) employment adaptation, explained by the independent variable, caregiver involvement (total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care). Discriminant functional analyses provided data to assess the contribution of the independent variable caregiver involvement (total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care, and hours of supervision) to Type II and Type III adaptations. The assumptions and interpretation for both regression and discriminant analyses remain as stated. The regression equation for Time 1, Type I is: Y=a+bX1+bX2+e where Y = employment adaptation a = the intercept between employment adaptation and the caregiver involve- ment variables b = the increase or decrease in Y for a one-unit change in X X1 = caregiver total involvement (ADL, IADL, and HCA) X2 = hours of physical care e = residual error 90 The linear discriminant equation for both Type H and Type III adaptation is: where D = the dependent variable, Type II (or Type III) employment adaptation B = coefficients, estimated from the data X = values of the independent variables X, = caregiver total involvement (ADL, IADL, and HCA) X2 = hours of physical care X3 = hours of supervision Time 2 data did not suggest the need for additional analyses with caregiver involvement and Type I or Type II adaptations. An independent t-test (two-tailed) provided data to assess the significance of mean differences in hours of supervision with Time 2, Type III (quit/retire) adaptation. In the present study, the equality of means for the hours of supervision spent by caregivers who chose to quit or retire at Time 2 and those who did not was the item of interest. Question 12: Which identified key variables from questions 4, 7, and 11 are the best predictors of each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? Type I = caregiver age, relationship to care recipient, caregiver gender, amount of family help, hours of physical care Type II = caregiver education, co-residence now, amount of family help, hours of physical care caregiver household income, family network, frequency of family help, total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care or caregiver education and hours of supervision) Type III As in Questions 4, 7, and 11, multiple regression and discriminant analysis provided data to answer this research question and its hypotheses. The variables selected for additional study using stepwise regression to analyze Time 1, Type I (maintain, but alter work) employment adaptation were those significant from the three preceding Time 1 regressions (caregiver age, caregiver relationship to the care recipient, care recipient 91 gender, amount of family help, and hours of physical care). To determine whether an alternate regression approach would generate different outcomes for this question, backward multiple regression also was used. With backward regression, all variables initially enter the equation and then are systematically removed based on pre-established criteria (Norusis, 1990b). The significant variables (caregiver education, co-residence now, amount of family help, hours of physical care) from the previous Time 1 discriminant analyses were used for further two-group discriminant analysis of Time 1, Type H (leave of absence) adaptation. Similarly, significant variables from the preceding Time 1 discriminant equations (family network, frequency of family help, total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care) or t-test analysis (caregiver household income) were used for more study of Time 1, Type III (quit/retire) adaptation. A review of Time 2 data did not suggest the need for additional analyses of Type I or Type II adaptations with any of the variables. However, significant variables from Time 2, Type 111 independent t-test analyses (caregiver education, hours of supervision) were used for additional investigation of Type III adaptation at Time 2. The regression equation for Time 1, Type I adaptation is: Y=a+bX1+bX2+..;bX5+e where Y = employment adaptation a = the intercept between Y and the independent variables b = the increase or decrease in Y for a one-unit change in X X1 = caregiver age X2 = caregiver relationship to care recipient X3 = care recipient gender X4 = amount of family help X5 = hours of physical care e = residual error where where where 92 The linear discriminant equation for Time 1, Type II adaptation is: D=B+BX1+BX2+BX3+BX4 D = the dependent variable, Type II employment adaptation B = coefficients, estimated from the data X = values of the independent variables X1 = caregiver education X2 = co-residence now X3 = amount of family help X4 = hours of physical care The linear discriminant equation for Time 1, Type III adaptation is: D=B+BX1+BX2+...BX5 D = the dependent variable, Type III employment adaptation B = coefficients, estimated from the data X = values of the independent variables X1 = caregiver household income X2 = family network X3 = frequency of family help X4 = total caregiver involvement X5 = hours of physical care The linear discriminant equation for Time 2, Type III adaptation is: D = the dependent variable, Type III employment adaptation B = coefficients, estimated from the data X = values of the independent variables X1 = caregiver education X2 = hours of supervision CHAPTER IV Results Study findings are presented in twelve sections that correspond to the research questions. Supported hypotheses required a p = < .05 level of significance. The first question asked whether employment adaptation occurred at Time 1 and Time 2; and, if so, if the adaptation type (I, II, or III) differed between the time periods. The probability of Time 1, Type I adaptation (maintain, but alter work) influencing Type 11 (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/ retire) adaptations at Time 2 received scrutiny through the second research question. The third, sixth, and tenth research questions queried the relationship of selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement with each type of employment adaptation. The fourth, seventh, and eleventh questions examined the variation in employment adaptation explained by caregiver and care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement. The fifth and ninth questions (specific for Time 2) asked about changes in the amount of family help and the measure of caregiver involvement over the past three months. The eighth question queried whether caregiving responsibility disrupted normal work and daily activities. Finally, the twelfth question asked about the influence of significant variables determined 93 94 from the answers to questions four, seven, and eleven on each type of employment adaptation. Study results for the questions and related hypotheses follow. Research Question 1 Does employment adaptation occur, and if so, does the adaptation type differ between Time 1 (inception) and Time 2 (three months)? First, frequencies or means and standard deviations for Time 1 and Time 2 employment adaptation variables were determined. Next, difference scores for each adaptation type were calculated. Finally, a paired t-test was utilized to examine Type I (maintain, but alter work), Time 1 and 2 differences, while the McNemar test (paired contingency tables) was used to evaluate the significance of differences for Type II (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/retire) employment adaptations. The caregivers in the study reported full-time (67.8%) or part-time (30.5%) employment status prior to assuming the caregiver role. At Time 1, the percentage of full-time employees dropped to 43.2% and the proportion of part-time workers declined to 21.2%. At Time 2, some caregivers (7.2%) reported additional employment changes (Figure 4). 95 .................... ................. .................................................... ..................................................................... ................................................................... ..................................................................... .............................. ................................................... .................................................................................... ................................................. Employed Employed Quit Leave of Retired Full Time Part Time Working Absence Figure 4. Time 2 Employment Status Changes (N=119) Also, work days lost due to caregiving responsibilities varied between pre Time 1 and between Time 1 and Time 2. At Time 1, caregivers recounted an average of 8.53 (SD = 17.52) lost work days (in the past three months); three months later (Time 2), caregivers reported a mean of 4.83 (SD = 15.96) for missed work days because of eldercare obligations (during the past three months). A paired sample t-test (two-tailed) revealed that the differences between the time periods were not significant. The mean for the index of Time 1, Type I adaptations (X = 1.75, SD = 1.58) declined for Time 2, Type I adaptation (X = 1.07, SD = 1.18). Thus, the mean dif- ference score (0.68) for Type I adaptation suggested less Type I adaptation at Time 2. The paired t-test confirmed significant differences (two-tail p = .000) between the time periods. The frequencies for Type II and Type III adaptations also dropped, implying 96 less use of those options during that period. Taking a leave of absence fell 11.0 percentage points (from 18.6% to 7.6%) and choosing to quit or retire from work declined 3.1 percentage points (from 16.5% to 13.4%). The McNemar Test confirmed significant differences (p = .002) for Type II, but not Type III adaptations between the time periods. Figure 5 contains the frequencies for the components comprised in the index of Type I adaptation (arrive late/ leave early, miss work without pay, take sick/personal days, change work hours, refuse a job/promotion, kept from looking for a job/better job, and other work effects) and the frequencies for Type 11 (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/retire) adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. The results revealed that each adaptation type occurred at both Time 1 and Time 2. 97 2.23323 EoE>oEEm .2 3.28339.“— .m 959“. _m>m_ 00. 3 3899.6 .cmo__.c9m u . ZO:b __wa>H .anhb 202E poo _ozwm .mSOI x5>> bag: 90.. 8.6805 x53 .35 _mcoflmd 3mm 305:5 .>:mm 9mm: 550 So: 58. 30.. mwnfim mocmco 22m mxmp «to; $3 \w_m_ w>_:< .................................. .................................. .................................. .................................. \\ .................................. .................................. ....... ..................... \ V ......................................... .................................. _ .................... m m m m m m \ m ...... m ..................... \ \ m ...... ....... ....... uuuuuuuuuuuuuuuu .............. . ..... m ..... - mm ..- .................................................... maize..- 9. won: oCHZ m. oEF. ommlz roEfiE om 98 Hla A variety of Type I employment adaptations occurs at both Time 1 and Time 2. The results supported this hypothesis. Caregivers reported a variety of employment adaptations (Type I index) at both Time 1 and Time 2 (Table 3). For both time periods, the most frequently recounted adaptation was arrive late! leave early (38.1% at Time 1; 23.5% at Time 2), while refuse job/promotion was the least often noted (18.6% at Time 1; 10.9% at Time 2). Hlb There are fewer Type I adaptive behaviors at Time 1 than at Time 2. The results did not support this hypothesis. Descriptive statistics suggested less Type I adaptation at Time 2 (Time 1, Type I X = 1.75; Time 2, Type I X = 1.07) than at Time 1. The paired t-test confirmed significant differences (p = .000) between Time 1, Type I and Time 2, Type I adaptation. While significant, the differences were not in the expected direction. ch There is less Type II adaptation at Time 1 than at Time 2. The results did not support this hypothesis. Descriptive statistics suggested more Type II adaptation at Time 1 than at Time 2 (Time 1 = 18.6%; Time 2 = 7.6%). The McNemar test confirmed significant differences (p = .002), but, as with Type I adaptation, not in the expected direction. 99 Hld There is no difference between Type III employment adaptation at Time 1 and Type III adaptation at Time 2. The results supported this hypothesis. Descriptive statistics suggested more Type III adaptation at Time 1 than at Time 2 (Time 1 = 16.5%; Time 2 = 13.4%). However, the McNemar test did not confirm significant differences between the time periods. Research Question 2 What is the probability that Time 1, Type I employment adaptation (maintain, but alter work) influences Time 2, Type II (leave of absence) and Type III (quit/retire) employment adaptations? Logistic regression determined the probability that Type I adaptation at Time 1 influences Time 2, Type II and Type III employment adaptations. The results of the procedure helped establish whether the model that included the variable in question (Time 1, Type I adaptation) told more about the outcome variables (Time 2, Type II or Type III adaptations) than a model that did not include the variable (model with only the constant). For Time 2, Type II and Type III adaptations, the -2 Log Likelihood (Type II, p = .999; Type III, p = .744) was not significant. The addition of Time 1, Type I adaptation did not indicate significant improvement, as indicated by the chi-square statistic, to the model with only the constant term for either Time 2, Type II (p = .618) or Type III (p = .812) employment adaptations. A partial correlation with the variables in both models was zero. Table 1 includes the logistic regression results for the influence of Time 1, TypeI employment adaptation on Time 2, Type II and Type III adaptations. 100 Table 1. Probability of Time 1, Type I Influencing Time 2, Type II and Type 111 Employment Adaptations Variable b Wald DF Sig Time 2, Type II (N =98) Time 1, .113 .252 1 .616 Type I Constant -2.527 17.403 1 .000 Time 2, Type III (N =99) Time 1, .043 .057 1 .811 Type I Constant -1.733 14.359 1 .000 Results from logistic regression provided the information to answer the following hypothesis. H211 The use of Time 1, Type I adaptation influences the use of Time 2, Type II and Type III adaptations. The results did not support this hypothesis. The logistic model that included Time 1 , Type I adaptation told no more about Time 2, Type II or Type III adaptations than a model with only the constant. The insignificant results indicated that Time 1, Type I employment adaptation did not influence the use of either Time 2, Type II or Type HI adaptations. 101 Research Question 3 What is the relationship of selected caregiver and care recipient characteristics to each type of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? caregiver age relationship to the care recipient caregiver marital status caregiver education caregiver occupation caregiver household income employment status (current part-time or full—time) co-residence status (now and before) care recipient age care recipient gender '-"=".=‘_o>E and mmcmmv Smd mmcmmv 2&6 mmcmmv 53350: 28350: .92 <0: .._o<_ ..o< .................... O 3355‘ mmmmmmm\ mum-LEV mmmmmmm\ .....\\ 3355‘ mam are m if, it in. an: \ may ....... smegma-m: \ in. if \m . m game mummy l fife El m 53323 \ ..HLJHH vmo Om . 3.3333\ m o vmmuom .......... i Hexnxxw mvouom mummhw ZNHOm :oiQm mum-”mm.- 33mm.- m ............ .9 - -. xz.a;-w _vslom oevlom mflmmm- W . .......... 8 . .......... B .fifififih U fume ............. imam-m. smr .................. «may». ow moiuom \ OFOPHQW mw—HZNmECD 0N ammuz _ 958 131 The specific approach and discussion for each hypothesis about caregiver involvement and employment adaptation follow. H10a Total involvement (ADL, IADL, and HCA) relates positively to employment adaptation. The results supported this hypothesis for Time 1, but not for Time 2. At Time 1, total caregiver involvement showed a significant positive relationship with each type of employment adaptation (Type I, r = .228, p = .000; Type H, r = .198, p = .008; Type HI, r = (.324, p = .000). However, Time 2 results revealed no significant relationships between total caregiver involvement and Type I (r = .135), Type H (r = .011), or Type HI (r = .113) adaptations. H10b Hours of physical care relate positively to employment adaptation. The results supported this hypothesis for Time 1, but not for Time 2. Significant relationships were evident with each adaptation type at Time 1 (Type I, r = .253, p = 000; Type 11 r = .250, p = 001; Type 111, r = .315, p .001). However, Time 2 results indicated no significant relationship with Type I (r = .115), Type H (r = .113), or Type HI (r = -.043) adaptations. H10c Hours of supervision relate positively to employment adaptation. The results partially supported this hypothesis for Time 1. Significant relationships were present with hours of supervision and Type H (r = 160, p = .035) and Type IH (r = .227, p = .001) adaptations, but not with Type I adaptation (r = .078) at Time 1. 132 At Time 2, only Type III adaptation showed a significant relationship (r = .343, p = .001) with hours of supervision. Type I (1' = -010) and Type 11 (r -085) adaptations did not have a significant relationship with hours of supervision. Research Question 11 How much variation in each type of employment adapta- tion at Time 1 and Time 2 is explained by caregiver involvement (activities and time)? a. total involvement b. hours of physical care 0. hours of supervision As in question 4, bivariate associations among the variables and each adaptation type were examined to eliminate from multivariate analyses potential explanatory factors that did not have significant associations. Stepwise multiple regression provided results to answer this research question and the related hypotheses for Time 1, Type I (maintain, but alter work) adaptation, while two-group discriminant analysis (Wilks’s Method) was used to determine whether caregiver involvement could differentiate those caregivers who used Time 1, Type 11 (leave of absence) and Time 1, Type HI (quit/retire) adaptations from those who did not. Total caregiver involvement and hours of physical care (the only variables significantly correlated with Time 1, Type I adaptation) were used in the regression equation. However, only hours of physical care met the criteria for the equation and explained 6% (R2= .064, p = .000) of the variance in Type I adaptation. At Time 2, no caregiver involvement variable demonstrated a significant relationship with Type I adaptation. Table 18 contains the regression results for caregiver involvement and Type I adaptation. 133 Table 18. Regression for Caregiver Involvement and Time 1, Type I Fanloyment Adaptation Variable b B T Sig t Constant 1.494 1 1.918 .000 Hours of physical care .076 .253 3.925 .000 R2 .064 F 15.403 p-value .000 N 228 Variables in equation = p < .05 Each involvement variable displayed significant relationships with Time 1, Type H and Time 1, Type IH adaptations; thus, all were used in the discriminant analyses. Chi-square analyses of the canonical discriminant function showed significance for the overall effectiveness of the model for both Type H (p = .001) and Type HI (p = .000) adaptations. The discriminant function correctly grouped nearly 66% (65.90) of the cases for Type H and almost 72% (71.56) for Type IH adaptations. Correlations and contingency tables did not reveal significant variables for Type H adaptation at Time 2; thus additional study with discriminant analysis was not done. Table 19 contains the canonical discriminant functions and Table 20 includes the discriminant classification results. 134 Table 19. Canonical Discriminant Functions for Caregiver Involvement Variables and Time 1, Type H and Type HI Adaptations Eigen- value Correlation Lambda x2 DP Sig Type 11 Time 1 .068 .252 .937 11.039 1 .001 (N = 171) Type IH Time 1 .137 .347 .880 28.229 2 .000 (N =223) Table 20. Discriminant Rmction Classification for Caregiver Involvement Variables and Time 1, Type H and Type HI Adaptations Predicted Predicted Type H (N = 171) Actual Group Cases LOA No LOA LOA 44 (100%) 16 (36.4%) 28 (63.6%) No LOA 129 (100%) 31 (24.0%) 98 (76.0%) Percent of cases 65.90% correctly grouped Type HI (N =223) Actual Group Cases Quit/Retire No Quit or Retire Quit/Retire 36 (100%) 22 (61.1%) 14 (38.9%) No Quit/Retire 189 (100%) 50 (26.5%) 139 (73.5%) Percent of cases 71.56% correctly grouped 135 Only one variable (hours of supervision) showed a significant relationship with Time 2, Type HI adaptation. Because there was only one involvement variable with a significant relationship to Type IH adaptation, an independent t-test (two-tailed) was used to check the significance of mean differences in hours of supervision with Time 2, Type IH adaptation. The independent t-test confirmed significant differences (two-tail p = .001) related to the hours of supervision expended by those caregivers who chose to quit or retire at Time 2 and those who did not. Table 21 presents the independent t- test outcomes for Type III adaptation at Time 2. Table 21. Independent t-test for Hours of Supervision and Time 1, Type HI Employment Adaptation Type HI Number of Cases Mean SD Time 2 16 12. 19 7.61 Quit/Retire Time 2 83 5.82 6.26 No Quit/Retire Pooled Variance t value -3.60 DF 97 2-tail probability Estimate .001 Caregiver involvement offered little explanatory power for Time 1, Type I adaptation (R2 = .063). The Time 1 discriminant analyses classification results were more impressive, especially for Type III adaptation (72% correct grouping). Type H adaptation (66% correct grouping) indicated less association with the caregiver involvement variables. The specific approach and discussion for each hypothesis about caregiver involvement and employment adaptation follow. 136 Hlla Hours of physical care explain more variation in employment adaptation than total involvement or hours of supervision at both Time 1 and Time 2. The results supported this hypothesis for Type I and Type H adaptations, but only partially for Type HI adaptation at Time 1. Hours of physical care was the only variable to meet criteria for the regression equation; thus, it explained more variation in Type I adaptation than either total caregiver involvement or hours of supervision. However, even though hours of physical care entered the equation, it contributed little (R2 = .064, p = .000) toward explaining the variation in Type I adaptation at Time 1. Similar results occurred with Type II adaptation. Hours of physical care was the only variable that met tolerance levels for entry into the discriminant equation. However, for Type HI adaptation, both hours of physical care and total caregiver involvement met the criteria for the discriminant function, indicating that both variables contributed to the differentiation of Type III adaptation at Time 1. Also, review of the standardized coefficients for the variables indicated similar results (Table 22). Table 22. Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Caregiver Involvement Variables and Time 1, Type H and Type HI Employment Adaptations Type II Type HI (N=171) (N=223) Unstand- Standard- Unstand- Standard- ardized ized ardized ized Hours of physical care .208 1.000 .107 .541 Caregiver total in- NA NA .386 .564 volvement NA Variable did not meet equation tolerance level 137 The hypothesis that hours of physical care would explain more variation in employment adaptation than total involvement and hours of supervision was not supported by Time 2 results. No involvement variable displayed a relationship with Type I or Type H adaptations. Hours of supervision was the only involvement variable with a significant relationship to Type IH adaptation. Thus, hours of supervision explained more variation in Type IH adaptation than did hours of physical care. Research Question 12 Which identified key variables from questions 4, 7, and 11 are the best predictors of employment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2? Type I = caregiver age, relationship to care recipient, caregiver gender, amount of family help, hours of physical care Type II = caregiver education, co-residence now, amount of family help, hours of physical care Type 111 = caregiver household income, family network, frequency of family help, total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care or care- giver education and hours of supervision) As in Questions 4, 7, and 11, stepwise multiple regression and discriminant analysis provided results to answer this research question and its hypotheses. The variables selected for the stepwise regression to analyze Time 1, Type I adaptation were those significant from the three preceding regressions (caregiver age and relationship to the care recipient, care recipient gender, amount of family help, hours of physical care). Three variables (caregiver age, hours of physical care, amount of family help) explained 19% (R2 = .186), p = .000) of the variance in Type I adaptation. Table 23 contains the regression outcomes. 138 Table 23. Regression of Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type I Adaptation Variable b [3 T Sig t Constant 3.215 7.047 .000 Caregiver age -.037 -.283 -4.549 .000 Hours of physical .064 .212 3.478 .001 care Amount of family .075 .160 2.570 .011 help R2 .186 F 16.892 p—value .000 N 210 Variables in equation = p < .05 Backward regression, using the same variables as in the stepwise regression of selected significant variables, was used to ascertain whether an alternate approach to analysis would generate different outcomes. With backward regression, all variables initially enter the equation and then are systematically removed depending on pre- established removal criteria (Norusis, 1990b). In contrast to the stepwise model, backward regression (Table 24) produced an equation that included all the variables (caregiver age, family relationship, hours of physical care, amount of family help and care recipient gender). This combination of variables and method produced a modest increase in the amount of Type I variance explained (R2 = .229). Table 24. Backward Regression of Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type 139 I Adaptation Variable b Constant 2.655 4.682 .000 Hours of physical care .057 .190 3.154 .002 Spouse relationship 1.445 .416 3.055 .003 Amount of family help .082 .175 2.670 .008 Caregiver age -.039 -.297 -3.911 .000 Care recipient gender -.700 -.219 -2.392 .018 Daughter relationship .881 .272 2.737 .007 R2 .229 F 10.841 p-value .000 N 210 Variables out of equation = p > .10 Previous discriminant analyses results provided the significant variables (caregiver education, co-residence now, amount of family help, hours of physical care) for additional two-group discriminant analysis of Time 1, Type H adaptation. This combination of variables produced a model with overall significance (p = .001). The discriminant function for the selected variables and Type H adaptation correctly grouped 64% (64.16) of the cases. For Time 1, Type III investigation, significant variables from the previous Type HI discriminant equations (family network, frequency of family help, total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care) and the independent t-test (caregiver household 140 income) were used in the discriminant analysis. These variables generated a significant overall equation (p = .000) and correctly classified 75% (75.25%) of the cases. Table 25 contains the canonical discriminant functions and Table 26 includes the classification outcomes for selected significant variables and Time 1, Type H and Type HI adaptations. Table 25. Canonical Discriminant Functions for Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type H and Type HI Adaptations 5‘53: 332$ 3.1%: x2 DP 5.. Type H Time 1 .124 .332 .890 19.776 4 .001 (N =173) Type HI Time 1 .178 .389 .849 31.764 5 .000 (N= 198) 141 Table 26. Discriminant Function Classification for Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type H and Type IH Adaptations Predicted Predicted Type II (N = 173) Actual Group Cases LOA N o LOA LOA 44 (100%) 22 (50.0%) 22 (50.0%) No LOA 129 (100%) 40 (31.0%) 89 (69.0%) Percent of cases 64.16% correctly grouped Type 1H (N = 198) Actual Group Cases Quit/Retire No Quit or Retire Quit/Retire 35 (100%) 24 (68.6%) 11 (31.4%) No Quit/Retire 163 (100%) 38 (23.3%) 125 (76.7%) Percent of cases 75.25% correctly grouped 142 In summary, selected significant variables (derived from the separate Time 1 regression and discriminant analyses results or the independent t-test outcome) for caregiver/care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement offered significant explanations for each adaptation type. The combination of significant variables differed for each adaptation type. Regardless of the variable mix, the outcomes suggest more explanation and differentiation power for Type HI adaptation (75% correct grouping) than either Type I (stepwise R2 = .186; backward R2 = .229) or Type H (64% correct grouping) adaptations at Time 1. At Time 2, there were no significant relationships identified with any of the caregiver/care recipient, family support, or caregiver involvement variables and Type I or Type H adaptations. Thus, additional analyses were not indicated for those adaptation types. However, two variables (caregiver education and hours of supervision) did have significant relationships with Time 2, Type HI adaptation and were used in two-group discriminant analysis. The two variables produced a model with a significant overall equation (p = .001) and correctly classified almost 75% (74.75%) of the cases. Table 27 contains the canonical discriminant functions and Table 28 the classification outcomes for selected significant variables and Time 2, Type HI adaptation. 143 Table 27. Canonical Discriminant Functions for Selected Significant Variables and Time 2, Type HI Adaptation Eigen- Canonical Wilks’s X2 DF value Correlation Lambda 813’ Type 111 Time 1 .171 .382 .854 15.170 2 .001 (N =99) Table 28. Discriminant Function Classification for Selected Significant Variables and Time 2, Type HI Adaptation Predicted Predicted Type III (N =99) Actual Group Cases Quit/Retire No Quit or Retire Quit/Retire 16 (100%) 5 (31.3%) 11 (68.8%) No Quit/Retire 83 (100%) 63 (75.9%) 20 (24.1%) Percent of cases 74.75% correctly grouped The specific approach and discussion for each hypothesis about key caregiver/care recipient, family support, and caregiver involvement variables and employment adaptation follow. 144 H1211 The hours of physical care are the best predictors of each type of employ- ment adaptation at Time 1 and Time 2. The results supported this hypothesis for Type H, but not Type I or Type III adaptations at Time 1. The discriminant analysis showed that the standardized canonical coefficient (Table 29) for hours of physical care (.659) was higher than for the other three variables in the equation (caregiver education, -.463; co-residence now, .304; and amount of family help, -.359). However, the stepwise regression analysis for Time 1, Type I adaptation (Table 23) indicated hours of physical care did not have the highest beta, but the second highest of the three variables in the equation (caregiver age, -.283; hours of physical care, .212; and amount of family help, . 160). Likewise, the backward regression (Table 24) showed that hours of physical care did not explain relatively more variation than the other variables in the equation (it was fourth highest of the five variables). Similarly, discriminant analysis for Time 1, Type HI adaptation displayed hours of physical care as having the second highest standardized coefficient of the five variables in the equation (Table 29). While hours of physical care was a good predictor of Time 1, Type I and Type III adaptations (coefficient = .429), the best predictor was total caregiver involvement (coefficient = .536). Table 29 contains the unstandardized and standardized canonical discriminant function characteristics for assorted variables and Time 1, Type H and Type III adaptations. 145 Table 29. Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Selected Significant Variables and Time 1, Type H and Type III Adaptations Type H Type HI (N = 173) (N = 198) Unstand— Standard- Unstand- Standard- ardized ized ardized ized Caregiver education -. 194 -.463 NA NA Co-residence now .640 .304 NA NA Amount of family help -.983 -.359 NA NA Hours of physical care .137 .659 .826 .429 Caregiver income NA NA -. 176 -.300 Family members NA NA .150 .323 within 50 miles Frequency of family NA NA -.444 -.212 help Total caregiver in- NA NA .363 .536 volvement Note: NA Variable not significant for adaptation type (based on previous discriminant equations). At Time 2, there were no significant relationships with any of the variables and Type I or Type II adaptations. Thus, additional analyses were not indicated for the caregiver/care recipient characteristics, family support, or involvement variables. While there were significant relationships with Time 2, Type HI adaptation, and subsequent analysis, the variables did not include the hours of physical care (Table 30). Table 30 contains the unstandardized and standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients for selected significant variables and Time 2, Type IH adaptation. 146 Table 30. Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients for Selected Significant Variables and Time 2, Type HI Adaptation Type 111 (N=99) Type In (N=99) Unstandardized Standardized Caregiver education -.206 -.473 Hours of supervision .129 .838 H12b The amount of family help at both Time 1 and Time 2 is less effective in predicting employment adaptation than are the other variables in the equation. The results supported this hypothesis for Time 1, Type I and Type IH adaptations, but not for Time 1, Type II adaptation. In the stepwise regression equation for Type I adaptation (Table 23), the amount of family support had a lower beta (.160) than the other variables in the equation, suggesting it contributed less to explain variation in Type I adaptation at Time 1 than did caregiver age (6 = -.283) and the hours of physical care (,8 = .212). Similarly, the backward regression outcomes showed the amount of family support as having the lowest beta of the six variables in the equation. The analysis for Time 1, Type III adaptation did not include amount of family help (no previous significant relationship), thus implying that the variable contributes little to predicting Type III employment adaptation. However, the standardized canonical coefficient (Table 29) for amount of family help was the third highest (-.359) of the four variables in the equation to differentiate Time 1, Type II adaptation. While the results indicated the variable was relatively less effective than two of the variables (hours of physical care coefficient = .659 and 147 caregiver education coefficient = -.463), co—residence now (coefficient = .304) offered the smallest relative contribution to the discriminant function. At Time 2, there were no significant relationships with any of the variables and Type I or Type H adaptations. While there were significant relationships with two of the variables and Type IH adaptation, they did not include the amount of family help. This suggests that the amount of family help offered little explanatory power for Type 1, Type H, or Type HI adaptations at Time 2. H12c Caregiver and care recipient characteristics explain more variation in employment adaptation than characteristics of caregiver involvement. The results supported this hypothesis for Time 1, Type I adaptation, but not for Time 1, Type H or Type III, or Time 2, Type III adaptations. The stepwise regression results (Table 23) indicated caregiver age (B = -.283) contributed relatively more than the other two variables (hours of physical care, B = .212; amount of family help, B = .160) to explain the variation in Type I adaptation at Time 1. Similarly, when backward regression analysis was used (Table 24), caregiver/care recipient characteristics (family relationship, caregiver age, and care recipient gender) explained relatively more of the variation in Type I adaptation than did the involvement variable (hours of physical care). However, for Time 1, Type II adaptation, hours of physical care had the highest standardized canonical coefficient (.659) of the four variables in the equation (Table 29). The lower standardized canonical discriminant coefficients of the two caregiver/care re- cipient characteristic variables in the equation (caregiver education = -.463; co-residence 148 now = .304) suggest they offered relatively less to the discriminant function than did an involvement variable. For Time 1, Type 1H adaptation, total caregiver involvement had the highest standardized canonical coefficient (.536), and hours of physical care had the next highest (.429) of the five variables in the discriminant equation (Table 28). This suggests that involvement variables had a relatively greater contribution to the discriminant function than did the only caregiver/care recipient variable to enter the equation (caregiver household income = -.300). At Time 2, there were no significant relationships with any of the variables and Type I or Type H adaptations. This suggests that both caregiver/care recipient characteristics and caregiver involvement offered little contribution to explain employment adaptation. However, Time 2, Type III adaptation displayed a significant relationship with a caregiver characteristic variable (education) and an involvement variable (hours of supervision). Discriminant analysis produced standardized canonical coefficients for the variables (education = -.473; hours of supervision = .838) that suggest that an involvement variable contributed relatively more to the equation than did the caregiver characteristic variable. Thus, the results do not support the hypothesis for Time 2, Type IH adaptation. CHAPTER V Discussion This section includes a summary of study findings, followed by discussion and implications for practice, policy, and research. The results of the analyses are organized by employment adaptation type and the relationship of each (Type 1, Type H, Type 1H) with the groups of independent variables. Outcomes at the .05 level were considered significant. The study, encompassing two time periods, broadened the scope of the usual employment and family caregiving literature in several ways. First, it focused on eldercare, not childcare, and employment. While substantial research about childcare and employment exists (Friedman, 1988; Kola & Dunkle, 1988), the study of the interaction of eldercare and employed caregivers is a relatively new phenomenon (Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, & Schoonover, 1987). Second, eldercare and employment adaptation were considered to be dynamic, not static, processes that were likely to change over time. Existing longitudinal studies (Given, Stommel, & Lin, 1991, Robinson & Thumher, 1979; Stoller & Pugliesi, 1989b; Zarit, Todd, & Zarit, 1986) present a complex picture of change. Finally, the researcher incorporated potential explanatory factors (caregiver/care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement) to assess their 149 150 influence on caregiver employment adaptations. The majority of existing family caregiver and employment literature is not only cross-sectional in design, but also, with some exceptions (Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, & Boise, 1990), descriptive and without an attempt to predict employment adaptation based on the presence of certain variables. Summary of Findings This study investigated the influence of three blocks of variables (caregiver/care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement) on using short-term employment adjustments, taking a leave of absence, and quitting or retiring from work in order to provide eldercare. Two time periods, inception of the eldercare role and three months later, were studied. At inception of the eldercare role, the caregiver’s age and relationship to the care recipient, whether she lived with the care recipient, the hours of physical care, and the care recipient’s gender were likely to influence the short-term employment adjustments made by the caregiver to meet family obligations. The family support characteristics did not seem to have a strong relationship with the temporary work alterations used by the caregiver. When the significant variables from each block were studied together, three variables, caregiver age, hours of physical care, and the amount of family help, were the most useful in describing the caregiver more likely to use short-term work adjustments in order to provide care. Caregivers who chose to take a leave of absence from their employment at inception of the eldercare role were influenced by slightly different factors. It appears 151 that a leave of absence may be more likely to occur with caregivers who have less education, live with their care recipient, and provide more hours of physical care than do those who choose to continue employment. The amount of family help provided to either the caregiver or the care recipient exerted a limited effect on the caregiver’s decision to take a leave of absence. However, when the four variables (caregiver education, co-residence, amount of family help, and hours of physical care) were studied together, they each were important in describing the caregiver likely to use a leave of absence in order to meet demands of eldercare. Finally, choosing to quit or retire from the workforce at inception of the eldercare role seems more likely to be the decision of a caregiver with a lower household income and who provides higher physical care hours. The family care network number, and the amount and frequency of family help appeared to have little influence on the decision of the caregiver to quit or retire. Nonetheless, when the family variables, household income, and hours of physical care were studied as a group, each was important in predicting caregivers more likely to quit or retire in order to provide care. The results for the second time period of study were very different. Three months after assuming the eldercare role, no caregiver or care recipient characteristic, family support, or caregiver involvement variable influenced the caregiver’s decision to make short-term work adjustments or to take a leave of absence from her employment. However, the caregiver who chose to quit or retire three months after assuming the eldercare role was more likely to be less educated and to provide more hours of supervision than did those caregivers who continued employment. A complete summary 152 of results for both inception of the eldercare role (Time 1), and three months later (Time 2) follows. 11019.1 Type I (maintain, but alter work) Employment Adaptation Time 1 results indicated seven caregiver/care recipient variables (caregiver age, household income, education, relationship to the care recipient, current employment status, co-residence now, and care recipient gender) had significant relationships with Type I adaptation. Four variables (caregiver marital status, occupation, co—residence before, and care recipient age) showed no significant relationship with Time 1, Type I adaptation. In order to assess the relative influence of the significant caregiver/care recipient characteristics on Type I adaptation, a regression analysis was done. Employment status, although significant, was not included in the analysis so that the impact of the remaining significant caregiver/care recipient characteristics could be calculated. Three variables, (caregiver age, relationship to care recipient, and care recipient gender), explained 19% (R2 = .191) of the variance in Time 1, Type I adaptation. From the group of three family variables (amount of family help, frequency of aid, and family network), only the amount of family help related significantly to Time 1, Type I adaptation. Although just one variable displayed a significant relationship with Time 1, Type I adaptation, all family variables were used in a regression analysis because of family’s importance in the conceptual model for the study. As bivariate 153 correlations suggested, amount of family help was the sole variable to enter the equation and explained just 5% (R2 = .047) of the variance in Type I adaptation. Two involvement variables (total caregiver involvement and hours of physical care) displayed a significant correlation with Type I adaptation, but one (hours of supervision) did not. Of the significant variables, only hours of physical care met criteria to enter the subsequent regression equation and accounted for 6% (R2 = .064) of the variance in Time 1, Type I adaptation. rype II (leave of absence) Employment Adaptation For Time 1, Type H adaptation, only three of the eleven caregiver/care recipient characteristic variables (caregiver education, current employment status, and co-residence now) displayed significant relationships. As with Type I adaptation, employment status was not used in subsequent multivariate analysis. The other significant variables (caregiver education and co-residence now) contributed to a significant discriminant function and correctly grouped almost 59% of the cases. However, that percentage is less than the identified practical significance (60%) for the study. Although just one family variable (amount of family help) showed significance with Time 1, Type II adaptation, as with Time 1, Type I analyses, all were used in the discriminant equation because of family’s importance in the conceptual model for the study. However, similar to Type I outcomes, only one variable, the amount of family help, contributed to the discriminant function and correctly grouped 55% of the cases. The low percentage of appropriate classification is explained by the discriminant model’s overall ineffectiveness with family variables. 154 Each involvement variable (total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care, hours of supervision) demonstrated a significant relationship with Time I, Type H adaptation. Nonetheless, only one (hours of physical care) contributed to the discrimi- nant function, produced a significant model, and correctly grouped 66% of the eases. Thus, hours of physical care appears to be a more effective variable to predict Type H employment adaptation when compared with the amount of family help. Type III (quit/retire) Employment Adaptation Only one of the eleven caregiver/ care recipient characteristics (caregiver household income) showed significance with Time 1, Type HI employment adaptation. An independent t-test confirmed significant differences related to the income of those caregivers who chose to quit or retire (lower mean incomes) and those who did not. Two of the three family variables (family network and frequency of family aid) showed significance with Time 1, Type HI adaptation. However, as with Type H adaptation, the variables did not generate a discriminant model with overall significance. Even though both variables contributed to the discriminant function, they correctly grouped only 57 % of the cases, not much better than the 50% expected by chance alone. As with Type II adaptation, each involvement'variable displayed significance with Type HI adaptation. However, only two (total caregiver involvement, hours of physical care) generated an effective model, contributed to the discriminant function, and induced improved classification results (72% correct grouping). 155 1111112 All Adaptation Types Time 2 results for Type I and Type H adaptations were inconclusive. No caregiver/care recipient characteristic, family support, or caregiver involvement variable predicted Type I or Type H adaptations. Also, no family support variable predicted Type IH adaptation. However, a caregiver/ care recipient variable (caregiver education) and. an involvement variable (hours of supervision) showed significant relationships with Type HI adaptation. Independent t-tests confirmed significant differences for each variable and Type HI adaptation. Caregivers who chose to quit or retire at Time 2 had a lower mean educational level and provided more hours of care recipient supervision than those caregivers who continued their employment. Compatibility with the Conceptual Model The conceptual model guiding the study placed equivalent emphasis on each group of independent variables and their influence on employment adaptation. Outcomes of statistical analyses related to Time 1, Type I adaptation did not support the model, however. At Time 1, caregiver/care recipient characteristics (R2 = . 191) evidenced stronger predictive power than either family support (R2 = .047) or caregiver involvement variables (R2 = .064). This outcome was incompatible with the preliminary conceptual model (Figure 1, p. 38). Type H results were more congruent with the model than Time 1, Type I effects. Although differences existed between the variable groups, they were less dramatic than 156 findings for Time 1, Type I adaptation. For Time 1, Type H adaptation, the caregiver involvement variables generated the highest correct grouping of cases (66%). Caregiver/care recipient characteristics were next with 59 % correct, while the family support variables had the weakest outcomes (55 % correct grouping). Time 1, Type HI differences, as with Time 1, Type I contrasts, were incompatible with the preliminary conceptual model. Classification variations for Time 1, Type IH adaptation, based on the three groups of independent variables, were less dissimilar than findings for Time 1, Type I, but more diverse than Time 1, Type H adaptation. Caregiver involvement generated the best (72% correct) grouping, family support produced the next highest (57% correct), while caregiver/care recipient variables had the weakest outcome (only one variable showed significance). The Time 2 data, particularly for Type I and Type H adaptations, did not support either the expectations for the study or the appropriateness of the conceptual model. For Type I and Type II adaptations, no variable from any independent group met criteria to enter the regression equation. While Type III adaptation did have improved outcomes, variables from only two of the independent groups (caregiver/care recipient characteris- tics and caregiver involvement) showed significance. The third variable group (family support) showed no relationship with Type III adaptation. Perhaps the general incompatibility of the model to employment adaptation at both time periods represents the uniqueness of each care situation. Multiple factors influence caregivers in varied ways and also are likely to change over time. The eldercare process and its interaction with work and family may not lend itself to a distinct conceptual model with equivalently influential variables. A model that includes all variables and 157 their significance (or lack) with each adaptation type and over time may offer more meaningful information about the unique influence each has on employment adaptation. A transitional conceptual model depicts key outcomes of the present study (Figure 8). 23329.0( .eeE>o_nEm a 2.202m 2.5.“. 3. .033 3:32:30 Eco—22.2... .o 2.3.“. 158 €998: oaoumo 8393 2352.38 35:63 E .50...ch om__o>c_ 00 .30» '''''' J - \ \ _ / \ t \ \ \ \ \ _ 7 9o: 2.83 .6539“. J 2 as: _ _ L \ \ \ r so: 2.83 .595 J llllll V \\ _i £0352 2.an J c3282 5 on: 50.6525 __ on: \ \ \ \ ‘ am < O k \ K ’I _ a J 1 llllll a I :05: oucoEmomoo . c9662? . u .. one; _ A :62. ooeoeaomoo \ — oan:. 00 J cessamo< _ ~53 7 3.59600 00 J \ .\ \ \ \ \ \ \ l i T 8:838 8 _ _ lllllll . »\ \\\\ \ \ i i l l i t. l l 7 3.2925200 i— _ 8:283. _ .i t t t T seaside 8 i— _ on; _ e ..A . _ o? 8 J r llllll gang gangs 159 V ' l r In the study, separate regression and discriminant analyses, plus independent t- tests examined the influence of three groups of independent variables (caregiver/care recipient characteristics, family support, and caregiver involvement) on each employment adaptation type. Significant variables from those procedures provided the independent variable list for a final query about their relative influence on employment adaptation. This approach incorporated the interaction of the independent variables that is central to Human Ecology theory and the employment adaptation outcomes that are important to Choice and Exchange theory. In the final Time 1, Type I regression equation, three variables (one from each independent variable group) explained about 19% (R2 = .186) of the variance for Type I adaptation. While caregiver age ()3 = -.283) made the highest relative contribution to the equation, it was closely followed by hours of physical care (,8 = .212). The amount of family help (B = .160) offered the least explaining power for the equation. When a backward regression (out = p > .10) was used with the same variables to see how results might differ, the R2 improved slightly (R2 = .229). The final equation included family relationship and care recipient gender as well as the variables that entered the stepwise version. In the backward regression, the caregiver/care recipient variables exhibited the highest betas (spouse = .416, caregiver age = -.297, daughter = .272, care recipient age = -.219). The amount of family help and hours of physical care had the lowest betas (.175 and .190, respectively). Thus, it appears that some caregiver/care recipient characteristics, using either regression approach, were 160 relatively more influential in predicting Type I employment adaptation than the other variable groups. Time 1, Type II outcomes of separate discriminant analyses also indicated that variables from each independent group were important in predicting Type II adaptation. The combined significant variables contributed to an effective discriminant model for Type H adaptation and correctly classified 64% of the cases. The best predictor variable, hours of physical care, had the highest standardized canonical discriminant function coefficient (.659) of the four variables in the equation (caregiver education = -.463, amount of family help = -.359, co-residence now = .304). Consistent with Time 1, Type I and Type II adaptations, Type III adaptation outcomes included significant variables from the three independent variable groups. Of the five variables in the final discriminant equation, total caregiver involvement had the highest standardized canonical discriminant function coefficient (.536). The other variables, hours of physical care (.429), family members within 50 miles (.323), caregiver income (-.300), and frequency of family help (-.212) offered relatively less to the discriminant function. The absence of significant relationships with any variable and Time 2, Type I and Type II adaptations undoubtedly depict disappointing Time 2 results. Only caregiver education and hours of supervision showed significant relationships with Time 2, Type III adaptation. However, those variables, representing two of the three groups of independent variables (caregiver/care recipient characteristics and caregiver involvement) contributed to the discriminant function and generated 75 % correct classification results. 161 The outcomes from the final analyses of significant variables from each independent group and their influence on the adaptation types led to the study’s concluding conceptual model. This model displays how, in relation to each other and over time, the variables influenced employment adaptation. Figure 9 shows the concluding conceptual model. 23:39.3 .eoanEEm a obtuse—um 2.5.“. .3 .302 33:00:00 255.230 .9 2:2“. 162 .8563 Smoumo 89.96 2962.36 .9... c. _So._.co.m 32.. All 8 9:5 2.5.2 827. O E>5m50u00 1. I 2235 c050 3955 c. _eau.__ca,.wn A l l l 2 95: 3:89.? r I. 5.652% A _ 5 25 8.2233 .0 2:0: \\ _ Emu _mo_m>cn_ _o 2:01 _ _ co=mamc<_ ll _cmEozoE. 00 .99. . 5 e9: _ 9m: 22%”. _o >ocm=cm£ _ $3.32 3m: 22:8 38:2 __ 8a 6‘ _ é x5352 2E5“. _ 1 llllll .— t _ . , eczmamg _ / \ _ _ > . / x x 5950 mo . . l l ..Imm .pl l l ’ \ \ 952v mocmoammoo 8:232 \ @885 Go _ 8.: x x x \ 8:828 00 \ \. . lllllll _ \ x l l a. 22mm _ CO——N—ch< - ' llllllllll .2 . _ m 00 m9 . A lllll _rll.l lpll. A 09.00 a? gag 163 In spite of the original conceptual model’s imperfect fit with study outcomes, the ecological approach to the study was important. The alteration of the model to reflect the perceived reality of the results illustrates adaptation, a basic tenet of ecological theory (Bristor, 1990). Also, the study provided a ”holistic way of viewing human systems as ecosystems with components bound together as functioning wholes in dynamic interaction with the environment" (Andrews, Bubolz, & Paolucci; 1980, p. 42). Illness or disability is not just an individual challenge, it involves the family caregiver, other family members, the employer, and ultimately society. Family caregivers respond, change, develop, act on information, and modify their environments, both to meet eldercare and employment responsibilities and also to manage other family and societal roles. Caregiver decisions, made in an environmental context, are used to attain individual and family goals (Bubolz & Sontag, in press). Conclusions The study shows that employed family caregivers used various employment adaptations to balance their work and family obligations. However, the particular predictor variables differed in their influence on the adaptation types. Diverse factors influenced caregiver decisions about both eldercare involvement and employment and, in turn, affected both time periods. The present study revealed some unanticipated results. The immediacy of the influence of acquiring the eldercare role on the employment adaptation of women represents a major finding. It was expected that increased employment adaptation (all types) would occur over time, specifically from Time 1 to Time 2. However, caregivers 164 did not wait until Time 2 to make adjustments. The percentage of caregivers who reported full-time or part-time employment prior to assuming the caregiver role related substantial changes at Time 1 (an average of 27 days after screening for entry into the study). Perhaps the Time 1 change (compared with prior employment) occurred in this retrospective study because the caregiver had no specific time frame to consider when responding to the question about employment prior to caregiving. Alternatively, the care for a recently physically impaired elderly relative may create a sudden need for a family member’s employment adaptation; thus, the caregiver decision to adapt work in order to contribute the required services. Regardless of Time 1 contrasts with reported prior (before assuming the caregiver role) employment, contrary to expectations, Time 2 changes showed no significant differences in the expected direction (increased accommodation) for Type 1, Type II, or Type III employment adaptations. Instead there were significant changes with Type I and Type II adaptations (less at Time 2) and there were no differences between the time periods with Type III adaptation. Perhaps this outcome represents a return to the caregivers’ balance of work and family obligations that existed before assuming the eldercare role. It also may mean the caregiver is adapting to the eldercare role so successfully, she finds it less necessary to adapt her employment. Clearly, the level of Time 1, Type I adaptation (maintain, but alter work) did not influence the use of either Time 2, Type 11 (leave of absence) or Type III (quit/retire) adaptations. While the absence of significant increased employment adaptation at Time 2 may be due to the considerable adaptation that occurred immediately after assuming the eldercare role, it also may reflect the study sample. A review of the reasons for the 165 decline in sample size at Time 2 reveals that 40 (17.9%) care recipients no longer required care at Time 2. Some care recipients, because they were physically, not mentally impaired, likely had conditions amenable to improvement through physieal therapy or some other rehabilitative method. In addition, 25 (11.2%) care recipients expired and four (1.8%) were either hospitalized or institutionalized at Time 2. Thus, some care recipients who presumably had high care demands (hence, more likely to influence employment adaptation) were no longer in the study. Conceivably, the Time 2 decline in employment adaptation may reverse at later periods of the core study (the complete study includes 6 time intervals, 3 months apart). Two snapshots in time (Time 1 and Time 2), although better than a true cross-sectional design, do little to assess the daily, weekly, monthly, or yearly adjustments likely to be part of the eldercare process. As noted, existing longitudinal studies found caregiving to be a complex process that changes over time. Robinson and Thurnher (1979), through their five-year qualitative study of adult children and their aging parents, found that the process of eldercare included a series of phases. Periods of increased caregiver tension, anxiety, and feelings of restriction characterized these stages. While the Robinson and Thumher study did not address caregiver employment issues, it is conceivable that similar patterns related to work may emerge and trigger increased adaptation. Caregiving is dynamic, not static; thus, intervals of varied intensity of adaptation may be representative of the process. A second unexpected study outcome is the negligible influence family variables exerted on employment adaptation at either time period. One possible explanation is the 166 measurement of family involvement. The instrument used to assess the frequency of family help had an acceptable alpha (.69). However, the tool utilized to learn the amount of family help may be less reliable. For example, the caregiver was asked to indicate the amount of help received from each network member. There may be no relationship between the amount of reported help from family member #1, from member #2, from member #3, and so on. Thus, the instrument may be less useful for obtaining important information about the family. Also, the self-report method of gathering information used with the core study, provides subjective, not objective data. Caregivers may vary in how they interpret the amount and frequency of help they receive from family members. Their assessment may be clouded by the way they perceive the family member in question. For example, a positive relationship may influence the caregiver to report substantial and frequent help from a family member whether it is objectively true or not. Conversely, the reverse situation also may occur. A negative relationship may sway the caregiver to assess the frequency and amount of help as being poor, while in actuality, the opposite may be true. The caregiver’s perception is her reality. Former family relationships, values, structure, and patterns of care or mutual support also may influence the involvement (or its lack) of network members with eldercare. Family care is a life cycle event, beginning with childcare (for most families) and ending with eldercare. A caregiver role for one individual may be sustained through varied family stages. Consequently, substantial eldercare commitment from the extended family may come only when the primary caregiver can no longer manage to provide 167 continued care. Studies show that one family member, usually a woman, provides the most care (Finley, 1989; Horowitz, 1985). Throughout the care situation, costs and exchanges occur. The caregiver’s interpretation of the costs and benefits of eldercare and/or employment adaptation determines whether or not either activity is seen as a problem. Walker, Martin, and Jones (1992) found that the costs of caregiving for daughters are reduced when there is a good relationship with their relative, if the care recipient lives in separate housing, and if the care recipient has fewer needs. While the present study included review of co- residence and caregiver involvement, it did not investigate the quality of the care- giver/care recipient relationship or caregiver burden. Young and Kahana (1989) found employment to be a significant predictor of adverse reactions (costs) during the caregiving period. Other researchers reported emotional strain, restrictions on time and freedom, and economic hardships (Buglass, 1989; Horowitz, 1985), as well as physical and mental exhaustion (Hooyman & Ryan, 1987). Another probable eldercare cost encompasses the hours of care expended by the caregiver. Care hours for the present study included separate categories for physical care and supervision. While the literature reports care hours without differentiating care rendered, studies document the consequences of those hours (Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, & Schoonover, 1987; Scharlach, 1989). For example, the research findings of Brody, Kleban, Johnsen, Hoffman, and Schoonover (1987) suggested that caregivers providing about 38 care hours per week were more likely to quit employment than caregivers with less time committed to care. 168 In the present study, there were significant positive correlations with both hours of physical care (about 26 hours/ week) and hours of supervision (about 49 hours/week) and Time 1, Type II adaptation (leave of absence) and Time 1, Type III (quit/retire) adaptation. A stronger relationship with hours of supervision (about 51 hours/week) existed with Time 2, Type III adaptation. Discriminant analysis showed that hours of supervision (along with caregiver education) correctly grouped almost 75 % of Time 2, Type III cases. Additionally, work days off without pay (a variable in the index of Type I adaptation) can be a caregiver cost. However, the measure may be biased because of the caregiver’s occupation and employment rank. For example, a professional or salaried employee may take a day off for eldercare without losing compensation (more schedule flexibility), while an hourly employee probably will not have that option. The same situation may occur with Type 11 (leave of absence) adaptation. Perhaps the leave is paid, but it may not be. As with Type I adaptation, a professional or salaried caregiver may have more options related to Type II adaptation than one who is an hourly employee. The prevalent work environment, policies, and procedures affecting work and family issues are not known for the study sample. In general, some employers have responded to family needs in the workplace by adjusting policies, benefits, or services (Buglass, 1989; Fernandez, 1990; Kola & Dunkle, 1988; Scharlach, Lowe, & Schneider, 1991), while others have not. Consistent with other studies (Archbold, 1983, Scharlach & Boyd, 1989, Walker, Martin, & Jones, 1992), caregivers at both time periods reported disruption with their lives and daily activities. In time, most caregivers adjust to their eldercare role, but the 169 eventual equilibrium shifts with the care recipient’s next period of change. In this study, counter to expectations, the adjustment included less employment adaptation at Time 2 than at Time 1. However, adaptation (regardless of the form) provides needed stabilization for the family and the caregiver with multiple employment and family obligations. In addition to the costs that conflicting work and family obligations may create, some researchers report benefits for employed caregivers. Enright and Friss (1987) related that employed caregivers of brain-impaired adults found work made their caregiving obligations easier. Researchers also reported that, for some caregivers, employment provided a psychological release and improved caregiver well-being (Barnes, Given, & Given, 1991; Brody & Schoonover, 1986; Enright & Friss, 1987; Stoller & Pugliesi, 1989a). Thus, employment may provide not only respite from family obligations, but it may also generate feelings of self-esteem because of job related knowledge and expertise. In contrast, caregivers may lack the needed skills and competencies for some eldercare tasks and, as a result, experience feelings of inadequacy. Caregivers frequently are unprepared for the management of conditions and administration of treatments formerly controlled in an acute care setting. It is possible that people continue working in order to limit the time available for caring. Further, varied reactions to caregiving and employment adaptation may relate to socioeconomic status, family values, and personal and family goals. The flow of resources (monetary, education, culture) and information (perception, values, 170 competencies) leads the caregiver to the outcome, decisions about family care involvement and employment adaptation. Implications Consistent with other studies (Creedon, 1987; Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, & Boise, 1990; Scharlach & Boyd, 1989; Stone & Short, 1990), data from the present research showed that employment adaptation occurs for employed caregivers. While the present study outcomes indicated less employment adaptive behavior at Time 2 than at Time 1, adaptations of all types still were evident. Also, this finding may illustrate that the caregiver has truly "adapted" to the eldercare role, made lifestyle modifications, and no longer finds it necessary to alter employment. However, the long- term effect of eldercare may influence continued fluctuation of employment adaptation. Thus, important ramifications for practice, policy, and additional research become evident. Implications for employee assistance personnel, family educators, and family professionals follow. In the workplace, persons who work directly with employee assistance programs (EAP) may need to augment their own knowledge of gerontology as well as increase their understanding of employed caregiver issues and possible solutions. BAP staff should anticipate the immediacy of employee’s responses to a family care crisis and be prepared for potentially rapid involvement. Long term measures also may be essential. In addition, EAP personnel may co—ordinate caregiver 171 fairs to highlight formal resources available in the community or offer educational sessions dealing with key issues. Similarly, EAP staff may sponsor ongoing programs that increase management’s realization of possible altered work productivity, utilization of caretaker benefits (family leave), and related costs because of conflicting work and eldercare demands. With enhanced appreciation of the potential influence family eldercare has on employment, managers may be encouraged to seek resolutions for work/family problems. A more responsive and flexible work environment for the family caregiver may evolve. Some organizations have recognized and responded to eldercare’s significance for their company. These pioneers for work-friendly environments for employees with family care responsibilities include: Travelers Insurance Companies, Bank of America, IBM, and Phillip Morris (Scharlach, Lowe, & Schneider, 1991); Southwestern Bell and Remington Products (Kola & Dunkle, 1988); Aetna Life and Casualty, Marriott (Buglass, 1989); and Chase Manhattan (Fernandez, 1990). A second practice implication involves family educators in academic settings. The educational preparation of family professionals requires content about family eldercare and its possible ramifications (employment and others). While persons directly involved in the care situation are cognizant of the costs (and the benefits), others may not be. As demographic and societal changes continue, work/family interactions will be increasingly important considerations in curriculum decisions. Knowledgeable instructors also should increase societal awareness of family eldercare and its implications to the employed caregiver juggling multiple roles. Forums on key issues (eldercare, employee needs, decision-making, values) offer one way to 172 reach a large audience. Persons attending such sessions may develop enhanced understanding and support for co-workers coping with the dual work and family roles. Family professionals represent a third group with practice implications derived from the work/eldercare interaction. Persons working directly with either primary caregivers or other family members may be resource brokers or providers of socio- emotional services (education, counseling, stress management programs, facts about community resources). Information about eldercare and resources available should be accessible to both the caregiver and other family members. Families might need encouragement to get involved with eldercare; conversely, the primary caregiver may require permission and support to allow other family members care responsibility. While the present study did not address whether or not the caregiver wished to have more family assistance, clearly, little family help was provided. The support available and the perception of the usefulness of the aid makes a difference in the caregiver’s interpretation of the amount and frequency of family help. The family professional may need to work with network members to improve communication skills so that the family is able to recognize available help and use it as needed. Exploring the caregiver’s willingness to provide care also may provide information about alternatives and potential substitute caregivers or other resources. In addition, the differential effects of any intervention must be considered by the family professional. What is helpful to one caregiver may not be to another. It may be necessary to make a concerted effort to change family task norms. For example, men can be encouraged to have more involvement in family care activities throughout the family life cycle. Intervention must be considered in the context of the caregiver and 173 family environment (past, present, future, psychological, and physical) to help identify the interdependence of the family and how that relates to the family care situation. Finally, family professionals may need to increase the flexibility of available service hours to meet the schedule needs of the employed caregiver. For instance, offering early morning, evening, or Saturday hours to provide the services required by either the caregiver or care recipient may allow the caregiver to meet family obligations without the need to adapt employment. Implicag’gns for Policy Although the present study used a relatively small sample and has some limitations, results provide additional information to help policy makers examine the issues related to programs (existing or proposed) designed to help employees with family care. The analysis of informal family care (eldercare or other dependent care) provides information to help restructure social arrangements and economic resources. Policy makers may need help altering the traditional image of "family" to one that is more realistic in today’s world. Often the caregiver is single (25% of the caregivers in the present study were unmarried) and many times the caregiver is from the ”sandwich” generation (61% of the present study were daughters, the average age was 52 years) and may have dependent children as well as responsibility for parent care (Brody, 1985; Sidel, 1986). Policies should not jeopardize the caregiver who chooses to take a leave of absence or leave the workforce temporarily in order to provide care. Currently, either adaptation (Type II or Type III) may jeopardize the caregiver’s own 174 elder years because of the probable interruption in health and pension benefits resulting from that decision. Flexible policies to meet the changing needs of family care throughout the life cycle are needed. While 31 states and the District of Columbia have some version of a family leave statute (most include maternity or adoptive leave; some also incorporate other serious family problems), a national policy does not exist (”The Family and Medical Leave Act,” 1991). Initiatives to establish national policies related to work and family should be nurtured and supported. Hooyman (1990) maintains that the well-being of family caregivers has not been the target for public policy. In contrast, Hokenstad and Johansson (1990) report that Sweden recently incorporated a care leave policy into the Swedish Social Insurance System enabling Swedish family caregivers to take up to 30 days paid leave from their employment in order to manage eldercare obligations. The Swedish government also emphasizes the provision of caregiving salaries if caregiving is a part-time or full-time job (Hokenstad & Johansson, 1990). While policy changes require immense effort and enormous popular and governmental support, they begin with individual involvement at the local level. This commitment eventually extends to state and federal arenas. Ultimately, policy changes supportive of work and family obligations will make a difference to the employed providers of family eldercare, both today and in the future. The adaptation of current policies to support family needs across the life cycle may come slowly, but it will come. Adaptation means the modification of behavior, feelings, and ideas to meet changing demands and conditions. This applies to both political and family environments. 175 h sear h The final implications derived from the study are those for research. Additional research, using the present study design, can provide data to determine whether there are patterns in the ebb and flow of employment adaptation. Future investigation should include the quality of the caregiving dyad affiliation and the perceived burden (cost) of the caregiver in order to evaluate differences in the costs of eldercare that may result when the relationship quality varies. Although the present study extends existing research by considering the interaction of eldercare and employment over two time periods, a longer time frame is needed. Longitudinal investigations (such as the core study) more ably address work/family issues than do the cross-sectional studies that dominate the caregiving and employment literature or the two time periods of the present inquiry. The workplace has paid little attention to the problems employees may have with eldercare responsibilities (Warshaw, Barr, Rayman, Schachter, & Lucas, 1986). While managers are beginning to recognize the close connection of work and family, its effect is still unknown. More studies are needed that compare the employment adaptation and the work quality of employed family caregivers with workplace accommodations of non- caregiving employees (Neal, Chapman, Ingersoll-Dayton, Emlen, & Boise, 1990). Research also is needed to identify workplace characteristics that help employed caregivers remain on the job as well as analysis to evaluate the effectiveness of existing family benefit programs (Scharlach, Lowe, & Schneider, 1991). Investigation to assess the linkage with eldercare and other work-related events (job turnover, productivity) provides yet another area of nwded research. Finally, 176 research that considers race and ethnicity and the diversity that those variables may bring to employed caregivers’ (both men and women) employment adaptation is required (White-Means & Thornton, 1990). This study investigated the influence of acquiring the family caregiver role on female employment adaptation at the inception of the eldercare function and three months later. The primary caregivers shared their time, their talents, and sometimes their home environment to meet eldercare obligations. They used internal resources (knowledge, skills, and coping abilities) to manage the physical, instrumental, and health care activities of daily living required by the care recipient. They altered work patterns and career goals. Caregivers at both time periods elected to use a variety of Type 1, Type II, or Type III adaptations in order to balance work and family care responsibilities. How that equilibrium will shift remains for future research to describe. LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES American Association of Retired Persons. (1987). Caregivers in the work place. Survey results. Overall summary. Washington DC: AARP. Anastas, J. W., Gibeau, J. L., Larson, P. J. (1990). Working families and eldercare: A national perspective in an aging America. Journal of the National Association of Social Workers, 35(5), 405-411. Andrews, M. P., Bubolz, M. M., & Paolucci, B. (1980). Study of the family. Marriage and Family Review, 3(1/2), 29-49. Archbold, P. G. (1983). Impact of parent caring on women. Family Relations, 32: 39- 45. Babbie, E. (1989). The practice of social research (5th ed). Belmont, CA: Wads- worth. Barnes, C. L., Given, C. W., & Given, B. A. (1991). Parent caregivers: A compari- son of employed and non-employed daughters. Manuscript submitted for publication. Barusch, A. S. & Spaid, W. M. (1989). Gender differences in caregiving: Why do men report greater burden? The Gerontologist, 29(50), 667-675. Bristor, M. W. (1990). Individuals, families and environments. Dubuque, IA: Kendall- Hunt. 177 178 Brody, E. M. (1985). Parent care as a normative family stress. The Gerontologist, 25(1), 19-29. Brody, E. M. (1990). Women in the middle: Their parent care years. New York: Springer. Brody, E. M. & Brody, S. J. (1989). The informal system of health care. In F. Eisdorfer, D. A. Kessler, & A. N. Spector (Eds.) Caring for the elderly: Reshaping health policy (pp. 259-277). Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press. Brody, E. M. & Schoonover, C. B. (1986). Patterns of parent care when daughters work and when they do not. The Gerontologist, 26, 372-381. Brody, E. M., Kleban, M. H., Johnsen, P. T., Hoffman, C. & Schoonover, C. B. (1987). Work status and parent care: A comparison of four groups of women. The Gerontologist, 27(2), 201—208. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bubolz, M. M., Eicher, J. B., & Sontag, M. S. (1979, spring). The human ecosystem: A model. Journal of Home Economics 28-31. Bubolz, M. M. & Sontag, M. S. (in press). Human ecology theory. In P. Boss, W. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W. Schuman, S Steinmetz (Eds.) Sourcebook of family theories and methods: A contextual approach, Chapter 17. New York: Plenum Press. Buglass, K. (1989). The business of elder care. American Demographics, 32-39. 179 Bureau of National Affairs. (1989). 101 key statistics on work and the family for the 1990s, Special Issue #21. Washington DC: Bureau of National Affairs. Craft, J. L. (1990). Statistics and data analysis for social workers (2nd ed.). Itasca, IL: F. E. Peacock. Creedon, M. A. (1988). The corporate response to the working caregiver. In Aging. Washington DC: US. Government Printing Office. Denton, K., Love, L. T., & Slate, R. (1990). Eldercare in the ’90s: Employee responsibility, employer challenge. The Journal of Contemporary Human Services, 349-359. Eldercare benefits meet new employee needs. (1988). Employee Benefit Plan Review, 42(12), 21,24. . Enright, R. B. (1991). Time spent caregiving and help received by spouses and adult children of brain-impaired adults. The Gerontologist, 31(3), 375-383. Enright, R. B. & Friss, L. (1987). Employed caregivers of brain-impaired adults: An assessment of dual roles. San Francisco: Family Survival Project. Feldstein, P. J. Health care economics (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley and Sons. Fernandez, J. P. (1990). The politics and reality of family care in corporate America. Lexington, Mass: Lexington Books. Ferree, M. M. (1987). Family and job for working class women: Gender and class systems seen from below. In N. Gerstel & H. E. Gross (Eds.), Families and Work, (pp. 289-301). Philadelphia: Temple University Press. 180 Finley, N. J. (1989). Theories of family labor as applied to gender differences in caregiving for elderly parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51(1), 79- 86. Fitting, M., Rabins, P., Lucas, M. J., & Eastham, J. (1986). Caregivers for dementia patients: A comparison of husbands and wives. The Gerontologist, 26(3), 248- 252. Franklin, S. T. (1991, November). The impact of acquiring the caregiver role on employment responsibilities of female caregivers. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, San Francisco, CA. Friedman, D. E. (1986). Eldercare: The employee benefit of the 19908? Across the Board, 45-51. Friedman, D. E. (1988). Family-supportive policies: The corporate decision-making process. In F. E. Winfield (Ed.), The work and family sourcebook (pp. 101- 116). Greenvale, NY: Panel. Friedman, D. E. & Gray, W. B. (1989, October). A life cycle approach to family benefits and policies. The Conference Board, Perspectives #19, 1-6. Frone, M. R. & Rice, R. W. (1987). Work-family conflict: The effect of job and family involvement. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 8, 45-53. Fullerton, Jr., J. N. (1991). Outlook: 1990-2005: Labor force projections: The baby boom moves on. Monthly Labor Review, 11, 31-44. George, L. K. & Gwyther, L. P. (1986). Caregiver wellbeing: A multidimensional examination of family caregivers of demented adults. The Gerontologist, 25, 253-359. 181 Gibeau, J. (1988). Working caregivers: Family conflicts and adaptations of older workers. In R. Morris & S. Bass (Eds.), Retirement reconsidered. New York: Springer. Gilhooley, M. L. M. (1984). The impact of care-giving on caregivers: Factors associated with psychological well-being of people supporting a demented relative in the community. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 57(1), 35-44. Given, C. W. & Given, B. (1989). Caregiver responses to managing elderly patients at home (Grant #2 R01 AG06584). Rockville, MD: HHS National Institute on Aging. Principal Investigator, Charles W. Given. Given, 8., King, 8., Collins, C., & Given, C. W. (1988). Family caregivers of the elderly: Involvement and reactions to care. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 11(5), 281-288. Given, 3., Stommel, M., Collins, C., King, 8., & Given, C. W. (1990). Responses of elderly spouse caregivers. Research in Nursing and Health, 13(2), 77-85. Given, C. W., Stommel, M., & Lin, C. S. (1991, September 25-28). Impact of patients with cancer upon family caregiver reactions: A longitudinal study. Presentation at the National Symposium "Respite Care in Chronic, Life- Threatening, and Terminal Illness” sponsored by the American Institute of Life- Threatening Illness and Loss, The Foundation of Thanatology, New York, NY. Glass, G. V. & Hopkins, K. D. (1984). Statistical methods in education and psychology, (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Graaff, J. De V. (1990). Social cost. In J. Eatwell, M. Milgate, and O. Newman (Eds.) The new Pelgrave, (pp. 251-258). New York: Stockton Press. 182 Grosof, M. S. & Sardy, H. (1985). A research primer for the social and behavioral sciences. Orlando: Academic Press. Haber, D. (1989). Health care for an aging society: Cost-conscious community care and self-care approaches. New York: Hemisphere Publishing. Hokenstad, M. C. & Johansson, L. (1990). Caregiving for the elderly in Sweden: Program challenges and policy initiatives. In D. E. Biegel & A. Blum (Eds.), Aging and Caregiving Theory, Research, and Policy (pp. 254-269). Newbury Park: Sage. Hooyman, N. R. (1990). Women as caregivers of the elderly. In D. E. Biegel & A. Blum (Eds.), Aging and Caregiving Theory, Research, and Policy (pp. 221-241). Newbury Park: Sage. Hooyman. N. R. & Ryan, R. (1987). Women as caregivers of the elderly: Catch-22 dilemmas. In J. Figueira-McDonough & R. Sarri (Eds.), The trapped wo- man/Catch-ZZ in deviance and control (pp.l43-l7l). Newbury Park: Sage. Horowitz, A. (1985). Family caregiving to the frail elderly. In C. Eisdorfer (Ed.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics, (Vol. 5). New York: Springer. Hosmer, D. W. & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New York: Wiley & Sons. Hu, T., Huang, L., & Cartwright, W. S. (1986). Evaluation of the costs of caring for the senile demented elderly: A pilot study. The Gerontologist, 26(2), 158-163. 1,600 days go to elder care, survey finds. (June 6, 1988). National Underwriter, 92(23), (Property/Casualty/Employee Benefits), pp. 6, 88. Jacobs, P. (1987). The economics of health and medical care. Rockville, MD: Aspen. 183 Kantor, D. & Lehr, W. (1975). Inside the family. New York: Harper Colophon. Klecka, W. R. (1980). Discriminant analysis. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, series no, 07-019. Beverly Hills: Sage. Kola, L. A. & Dunkle, R. E. (1988). Eldercare in the workplace. The Journal of Contemporary Social Work, 569-574. Lewis-Beck, M. S. (1989). Applied regression: An introduction. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, series no, 07-022. Beverly Hills: Sage. Magnus, M. (1988). Eldercare: Corporate awareness but little action. Personnel Journal, 67(6), 19,23. Matthews, S. H. & Rosner, T. T. (1988, February). Shared filial responsibility: The family as primary caregiver. The Journal of Marriage and the Family 50, 185- 195. Montgomery, R. J. V. (1989). Investigating caregiver burden. In K. S. Markides & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Aging, stress and health (pp.201-218). New York: Wiley. Neal, M. B., Chapman, N. J., Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Emlen, A. C., & Boise, L. (1990). Absenteeism and stress among employed caregivers of the elderly, disabled adults, and children. In D. E. Biegel & A. Blum (Eds.) Aging and caregiving: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 160-183). Newbury Park: Sage. Norusis, M. J. (1990a). SPSS/PC+ 4.0 Base Manual. Chicago: SPSS. Norusis, M. J. (1990b). SPSS/PC+ Statistics 4. 0. Chicago: SPSS. Norusis, M. J. (1990c). SPSS/PC+ Advanced Statistics 4. 0. Chicago: SPSS. 184 Nye, F. I. (1979). Choice, exchange, and the family. In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & Reiss, I. L. (Eds.) Contemporary theories about the family: General theories/theoretical orientations (Vol. 2), (pp. 1-41). New York: The Free Press. Older Women’s League. (1989). Failing America ’s Caregivers: A Status Report on Women Who Care. Washington DC: OWL. Orodenker, S. Z. (1990). Family caregiving in a changing society: The effects of employment on caregiver stress. Family Community Health, 12(4), 58-70. Pohl, J., Given, C. W., and Given, B. A. (1991). Caregiver responses to managing elderly patients at home. Progress Report for Grant #2 R01 AGO6584, HHS National Institute on Aging. East Lansing, MI: MSU. Robinson, B. & Thumher, M. (1979). Taking care of aged parents: A family cycle transition. The Gerontologist, 19(6), 586-593. Rook, K. S. (1987). Reciprocity of social exchange and social satisfaction among older women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(1), 145-154. Sakauye, K. M. (1989). Ethnic variations in family support of the fiail elderly. In M. Z. Goldstein (Ed.) Family involvement in the treatment of the frail elderly (pp. 63-106). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press. Scharlach, A. E. (1989). A comparison of employed caregivers of cognitively impaired and physically impaired elderly persons. Research on Aging, 11(2), 225-243. Scharlach, A. E. & Boyd, 8. L. (1989). Caregiving and employment: Results of an employment survey. The Gerontologist, 29(3), 383-387. Scharlach, A. E., Lowe, B. F., & Schneider, E. L. (1991). Elder care and the work force. Lexington, MA: Lexington. 185 Scharlach, A. E, Sobel, E. L., & Roberts, R. E. L. (1991). Employment and caregiver strain: An integrative model. The Gerontologist, 31(6), 778-787. Sidel, R. (1986). Women and work. Women and children last: The plight of poor women in afiluent America (pp. 48-76). New York: Viking. Stoller, E. P. & Pugliesi, K. L. (1989a). Other roles of caregivers: Competing responsibilities or supportive resources. Journal of Gerontology, 44(6), 8231- 238. Stoller, E. P. & Pugliesi, K. L. (1989b). The transition to the caregiver role: A panel study. Research on Aging, 11(3), 312-330. Stommel, M., Given, C. W., & Given, B. A. (1990). Depression as an overriding variable explaining caregiver burden. Journal of Aging and Health, 2(1), 81-102. Stone, R., Cafferata, G. L., & Sangl, J. (1987). Caregivers of the fragile elderly: A national profile. The Gerontologist, 27(5), 616-631. Stone, R. I. & Short, P. F. (1990). The competing demands of employment and informal caregiving to disabled elders. Medical Care, 28(6), 513-526. The family and medical leave act. (1991, April). Congressional Digest, pp. 98-109. The Travelers Companies. (1988). The Travelers employee caregiver survey. In F. E. Winfield (Ed.), The work and family sourcebook (pp. 239-249). Greenvale, NY: Panel. Touliaros, J. & Compton, N. H. (1988). Research Methods in Home Economics. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press. 186 U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1986). Women and the American economy (Current Population Reports, Series P-23, No. 146). Washington DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Waldo, D. R., Sonnefeld, S., McKusick, D. R., & Amett HI, R. H. (1989). Health expenditures by age group, 1977 and 1987. Health Care Financing Review, 10(4), 111-120. Walker, A. J., Martin, S. S. K., Jones, L. L. (1992). The benefits and costs of caregiving and care receiving for daughters and mothers. Journals of Gerontolo- gy, 47(3), 8130-139. Warshaw, L. J., Barr, J. K., Rayman, 1., Schachter, M., & Lucas, T. G. (1986). Employer support for employee caregivers. New York: The New York Business Group on Health. White-Means, S. I. & Thornton, M. C. (1990). Labor market choices and home health care provision among employed ethnic caregivers. The Gerontologist, 30(6), 769- 775. Willett, J. B. (1988). Questions and answers in the measurement of change. In E. Z. Rothkopf (Ed.), Review of Research in Education (pp. 345—422). Washington DC: American Educational Research Association. Winfield, F. E. (1988). Workplace solutions for women under eldercare pressure. In F. E. Winfield (Ed.), The work and family sourcebook (pp. 275-284). Greenvale, NY: Panel. 187 Wisensale, S. K. & Allison, M. D. (1988). An analysis of 1987 state family leave legislation: Implications for caregivers of the elderly. The Gerontologist, 28(6), 779-785. Young, R. F. & Kahana, E. (1989). Specifying caregiver outcomes: Gender and relationship aspects of caregiver strain. The Gerontologist, 29(5), 660-666. Zarit, S. H., Todd, P. A., & Zarit, J. M. (1986). Subjective burden of husbands and wives as caregivers: A longitudinal study. The Gerontologist, 26(3), 260-266. APPENDICES APPENDIX A APPENDIX A Instrument Caregiver Employment Questionnaire (selected items) 1. Were you employed outside the home preyiqus to your caregiving responsibility? _ YES (1) _ N0 (2) if yes, _ full-time (1) _ part-time (2) Note: At Time 2, the caregiver is asked if there has been a change in employment status in the last three months and if so, how it has changed. 2. What is your current employment status? employed full-time (l) employed part-time (2) self-employed (3) retired (4) leave of absence (5) unemployed; laid off/between jobs/disability (6) not employed for pay outside the home; homemaker (7) quit work to care for (8) 3. There are some ways that caregiving can affect or interfere with your work or employment. Please answer "yes" or " no for each item W currenflx malarial Has caregiving affected or interfered with your work: a. Caused you to be late for work/or leave work early? _ YES (1) _ NO (2) 188 4. 189 b. Caused you to miss days of work without pay? _ YES (1) __ N0 (2) c. Caused you to take sick/personal days? _ YES (1) _ NO (2) d. Caused you to change the hours you work (shift)? __ YES (1) _ NO (2) e. Caused you to turn down a job or promotion? _ YES (1) __ NO (2) f. Kept you from looking for a job, or a better job? _ YES (1) _ NO (2) g. Affected your work in other ways not mentioned? __ YES (1) _ NO (2) Describe: h. Caused you to take a leave of absence? _ YES (1) _ NO (2) i. Caused you to quit or take early retirement? _ YES (1) _NO (2) In the last three months, how many days of work have you missed in order to eare for your relative? (Write in Number of Days) __ Not Applicable 190 Sociodemographic Information . Caregiver is: _ Male (l)_ Female (2) Patient is: _ Male (l)_ Female (2) Caregiver Birth Date _ month _ day _ year Patient Birth Date _ month __ day °__ year . What is your (caregiver) marital status? (CHECK ONE) _ Single, never married (1) _ Married/ remarried (2) _ Divorced (3) _ Widowed (4) _ Separated (5) . What is your relationship to ? You are his/her: (Check one) _ Spouse (1) _ Parent (2) Daughter/Son (3/4) Daughter-in-law/Son-in-law (5/6) Sister/Brother (7/ 8) Sister/brother-in-law (9/10) Granddaughter/ son (1 1/12) Niece/Nephew (13/14) _ Aunt/Uncle (15/16) _ Other relative (l7) (specify) _ Friend or companion with whom you live (18) _ Other non relative (19) What is your highest level of education? (Check one) Completed grade school or less (1) Completed some high school (2) Completed high school (3) Completed some college or technical training (4) Completed college (5) Completed some graduate or professional school (6) Completed graduate or professional degree (7) . Do you and 191 currently live in the same household? (If "yes", caregiver was asked about prior) Yes (1) _ No (2) . Considering all sources of income for all members of your household, please indicate your gross income, before taxes, as you would report on your Federal Tax Forms. (Select one category) __ 0 - 1,999 (l) _ 22,000 - 25,999 (9) _ 2,000 - 3,999 (2) _ 26,000 - 29,999 (10) _ 4,000 - 5,999 (3) _ 30,000 - 34,999 (11) _ 6,000 - 7,999 (4) _ 35,000 - 39,999 (12) _ 8,000 - 9,999 (5) _ 40,000 - 44,999 (13) _ 10,000 - 13,999 (6) _ 45,000 - 49,999 (14) _ 14,000 - 17,999 (7) _ 50,000 - 54,999 (15) _ 18,000 - 21,999 (8) _ 55,000 - 59,999 (16) _ 60,000 and over (17) 192 Family Network Questionnaire The next set of questions asks about the families of you and the person for whom you provide care. First, we’d like you to identify all living adult relatives of the patient. We’d like you to include parents, sisters, brothers, spouse, and children. For each relative, we’d like you to give us the person’s initials, the relationship to the patient, and how far away they live from the patient in approximate miles. (THE INTERVIEWER FILLS OUT THE FIRST THREE COLUMNS OF THE TOOL AT THIS POINT WITH THE INITIALS, RELATIONSHIP TO THE PATIENT, AND DISTANCE IN MILES FOR EACH PERSON IDENTIFIED AS A LIVING ADULT RELATIVE OF THE PATIENT) Next, I’d like you to think about each one of these people in terms of how close they are to the patient and how close they are to you. By closeness, we mean the strength of the emotional tie between the patient and the relative, and you and the patient’s relative. Please rate the closeness of the relationship on a scale of 1 to 5 where 1 stands for a relationship that is not at all close and 5 stands for a relationship which is extremely close. Then I’ll ask you about how much help each of the relatives helps with caregiving. The answers you may choose from are NONE OR VERY LITTLE, A LITTLE, SOME, QUITE A BIT, or A GREAT DEAL. (THE INTERVIEWER CUES RESPONDENT WITH INITIALS AND RELATIONSHIP AND FILLS OUT THE REMAINING COLUMNS) CLOSENESS TO PATIENT CLOSENESS TO HELP PROVIDED CAREGIVER 1 = not at all close 1 = not at all close 1 = none or very little 5 = extremely close 5 = extremely close 2 = a little 3 = some 4 = quite a bit 5=agreatdeal 193 Only the number in the caregiver’s family network and the amount of help provided by each member was used in the study. At Time 2, the caregiver also is asked to estimate both the number of hours per week that each person assisted in caregiving and the total number of hours per week that others provided help. In addition, there are questions about changes in the help received (stopped or decreased and by whom; started or increased and by whom). Finally, the caregiver is asked whether help from the family increased, stayed about the same or decreased. 194 Social Assistance (of the family) Scale Now I’m going to ask you about the assistance that you receive from We to care for your relative (relationship or name of the patient). By other people, I mean friends or relatives whether you pay them or not. For the next set of questions, I will ask "How often since discharge, over the past month has anyone in your family given you assistance in the following ways," and I will read a list of activities. I would like you to please pick from one of four categories of responses: You can choose from "Rarely or None of the Time," ”Some of the Time," ”Most of the Time,” or "Almost all of the Time.” I will repeat those categories again if necessary. 1. How often since discharge or over tho past month, HAS ANYONE IN YOUR FAMILY given you assistance in the following ways: (CHECK ONE FOR EACH TASK) SCALE 1 = rarely or none of the time 2 = some of the time 3 = most of the time 4 = almost all of the time la. Help with physical care 1b. Spend time keeping your relative company 1c. Stayed with so that you could do something else for a few hours. 1d. Given YOU emotional support or encouragement 1e. Helped with transportation; for either you or If. Helped YOU with money or other material goods 1g. Checked on YOU to be sure that you were alright 1h. Provided/encouraged diversional activities for _such as cards or Scrabble Note: The core study also utilizes the questions to assess the assistance of friends. This study used only family responses. The questions are identical at Time 1 and Time 2. 195 Caregiver Involvement Instrument (selected items) The next set of questions addresses the PRESENT level of performance for the person you care for on a number of activities and the way YOU help him/her. For each item, please choose the response that most closely describes the patient’s PRESENT condition and how you help him or her. 1. DRESSING 1a. With regard to dressing, would you say ...(check one) _ IS INDEPENDENT _ NEEDS SUPERVISION ONLY _ NEEDS SOME PHYSICAL HELP _ NEEDS TOTAL PHYSICAL HELP _ IS NEVER DRESSED (always wears bed clothing) 1b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with dressing? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 2. EATING 2a. With regard to eating, would you say ...(check one) _ IS INDEPENDENT _ NEEDS SUPERVISION ONLY _ NEEDS SOME PHYSICAL HELP _ NEEDS TOTAL PHYSICAL HELP _ NOT APPLICABLE (needs tube feedings, IV’s) 196 2b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with eating? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 3. BATHING 3a. With regard to bathing, would you say ...(check one) _ IS INDEPENDENT _ NEEDS SUPERVISION ONLY _ NEEDS SOME PHYSICAL HELP _ NEEDS TOTAL PHYSICAL HELP 3b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with bathing? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 4. WALKING INSIDE THE HOUSE 4a. With regard to walking inside the house, would you say ...(check one) __ IS INDEPENDENT _ NEEDS SUPERVISION ONLY _ NEEDS SOME PHYSICAL HELP _ NEEDS TOTAL PHYSICAL HELP _ UNABLE TO WALK 197 4b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with walking? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 5 . TOILETING 5a. With regard to toileting, would you say ...(check one) _ IS INDEPENDENT _ NEEDS SUPERVISION ONLY _ NEEDS SOME PHYSICAL HELP _ NEEDS TOTAL PHYSICAL HELP _ NOT APPLICABLE (has catheter and colostomy) 5b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with toileting? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 6. TRANSFERRING IN/OUT OF BED 6a. With regard to transferring in/out of bed, would you say _ ...(check one) _ IS INDEPENDENT _ NEEDS SUPERVISION ONLY _ NEEDS SOME PHYSICAL HELP _ NEEDS TOTAL PHYSICAL HELP _ REMAINS BEDFAST 6b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with transferring in/out of bed? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 198 7. COOKING/ PREPARING MEALS 7a. How much help does presently nwd with cooking? Does he/she need: (check one) _ NO HELP _ SOME HELP _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate, but has done so in the past) _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate and never has) _ NOT APPLICABLE (patient has tube feedings, IV’s) 7b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with cooking or cook for him or her? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 8. HOUSEWORK 8a. How much help docs presently need with housework? Does he/she need: (check one) _ NO HELP _ SOME HELP _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate, but has done so in the past) _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate and never has) 8b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with housework or do housework for him or her? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY ' SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 199 9. SHOPPING 9a. How much help docs presently nwd with shopping? Does he/she nwd: (check one) _ NO HELP _ SOME HELP __ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate, but has done so in the past) _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate and never has) 9b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with shopping or shop for him or her? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 10. LAUNDRY 10a. How much help docs presently need with laundry? Does he/she need: (check one) _ NO HELP _ SOME HELP _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate, but has done so in the past) _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate and never has) 10b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with laundry or laundry for him or her? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 200 11. TRANSPORTATION 11a. How much help docs presently need with transportation? Does he/she need: (check one) _ NO HELP __ SOME HELP _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate, but has done so in the past) __ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate and never has) 11b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with transportation? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY 12. MONEY MANAGEMENT 12a. How much help does he/she need: (check one) presently need with money management? Does _ NO HELP _ SOME HELP _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate, but has done so in the past) _ TOTAL HELP (doesn’t participate and never has) 12b. How frequently do YOU help your relative with money management or do money management for him or her? NEVER ONCE A SEVERAL DAILY SEVERAL WEEK/LESS TIMES A WEEK TIMES A DAY Note: Responses are: 0 = never, 1 = once a week or less, 2 = several times a week, 3 = once a day, 4 = several times a day. In the core study, the involvement of other people (family and friends) is also assessed with this section. Tlnudu Ed (u ( (NI ( (I 201 The next questions deal with activities of health care (HCA). The caregiver is asked if the relative needs help with HCA. The caregiver indicates how frequently she helps with the required care recipient activities. Responses: 0 = never, 1 = once a week or less, 2 = several times a week (2-6), 3 = daily, and 4 = several times a day. URINARY CATHETER/CATHET ER CARE 0 1 2 3 OXYGEN ADMINISTRATION 0 1 IV, HICKMAN, BROVIAC CATHETER CARE/DRESSING 0 1 2 3 IV MEDICATIONS/FLUIDS/FEEDINGS 0 1 TUBE FEEDINGS OR IV FEEDINGS 0 l 2 3 INJECTIONS (PAIN MEDICATIONS, INSULIN) O 1 2 3 SPECIAL EXERCISES/ PHYSICAL THERAPY O I CARE OF ULCERS/BEDSORES O 1 2 3 SKIN CARE (SPECIAL CLEANSING LOTIONS) O l COLOSTOMY/COLOSTOMY CARE 0 I CARE OF POST OPERATIVE INCISION/WOUND ORAL MEDICATIONS NASOGASTRIC TUBE AND CARE INCONTINENCE OF URINE INCONTINENCE OF STOOL TRACHEOSTOMY/TRACHEOSTOMY CARE RESPIRATOR/CARE OF RESPIRATOR SUCTIONING OOOOOOOO r—tr—tr—sr—r—r—r—nr—A NNNNNNNN wmuuuwmw ##fi-fi-fihh-k Note: In the core study, this section also is used to assess the caregiver’s feelings of competency to help with the required procedures and to measure how often others help. 202 The next set of questions is about how much time you spend providing care for your relative. 1. PHYSICAL ASSISTANCE 1a. In a usual day how many hours do you spend providing physical assistance to your relative? By physical assistance, we mean physically helping your relative with activities such as eating, bathing, treatments, etc. (WRITE IN NUMBER) _ NUMBER OF HOURS/DAY 2. SUPERVISION TIME 2a. In a usual day (excluding night hours), how many hours do you spend supervising your relative? By supervision, we mean being with your relative to make sure he/she is safe (WRITE IN NUMBER) _ NUMBER HOURS/DAY Note: At Time 2, the caregiver also is asked to indicate if she is providing more care, less care, or the same amount of care compared to three months ago. APPENDIX B a -8. 42.. I Jew Itm I New. - --....a. .....a. let-ext- Hum. lit-me.- -i.e,.-.t l ..m...: -l 2.. www.91- fi 8.. 2.- 8.- 8.- .2. ..8. ..8. .2. .2. .2. 2.- 2.- .2: .o. 2.5 2 L ‘ 8.. ..fi. 8.- 8.- 8. ...8. ...8. ..fi. ..2. ..8. 8.- ..8. .2. 3.3.. 25...: _ 8.. .....n. ...a. 8. 8. 2. 2. 8. .2. ...on. ..2. 8.- :8. .... < .2 W 8.. 8. 8. 8. 2. 2. 8. 8. ...8. 2. 8.- 2. 32". a. fi 8.. 8. 8. ... ... :2. 8. 8.- 8. 8. n. .- E. ... ... _ 8.. ...S. ...8. .32.. ...8. ...on. 8. .2.- ..8: ... me: .2 _ 8.. ...S. ...8. :8. ...8. 8. :2.- ..2: 8.- 9. a: 0 “ V 8.. ...8. ...R. ...8. 8. 8. .2.- 8. .34. .. u 8.. ...3. ...... 8.- ...- 8.- 8.- <2. .- _ 8.. ...S. 2.- 8.- 2.- 8.- .9: .e . 8.. 8. 8.- ..o..- 8.- .9. .n 8.. ...... 8. ... on... ..o .4 8.. ...8. :8.- e58... .n 8.. .n..- B .N 8.. a? no .. v. n. a. .. 2 a a .. e m w ... u . ..E-p 8...: 8.3.2.5 m “Gamma. ... 25 u ... ......- ... 2.....- u .. ...... .. 2...... n ...... ...... ......a. .o .525 n ... < 383.2. 3.2.... u 32 n. .20.. 2.28 .8 55:3... H :m .n. ”c2232.... .0 2.5.. H m a... .28 32%.... ..o 8.5.. H Um 2.. 352023... .9328 .52 n 44.... ”823.2. 0.8 ...—.8.— " <0... $22. 2...... .0 822.2. 358.52.. n .55 35>: >3... .8 8.22.2. u AQ< ”own 2.228.. 9.8 .I- ow< m0 628... 823.8 u 2.82.. 2.2.833 533.8 u ..m ”own 829.8 fl ow< mo ”2.2.25.3... .89. H 3.2.. 3c. fl ...... ”no. H ... .302 8.- .... 2. we: .2 _ __ 8.. ...... ...... ...... ...... ...- 8.- .fi: 8. 9...: ... __ 8.. ...8. ....... ...»... 8. 2: 2.- 8. -.-.<... 8.. ...3. ...8. ...: .2: B.- 8. <2. .8 8.. 3.2.. 8.- 8.- 2.- 8. 3...... 8.. z. 8.- .2.- 8. .9. .n 8.. 2. 8. 8. a... mu .. 8.. ...u. :8.- .58... ... 8.. :8.- ..m .N 8.. ...... no. _— u. .. 2 a a .. o n v n a . «2...... _— APPENDIX C 205 8.2.22.3 .o 2.2. u m 2: .28 .882... ..o 2.5.. H Um 2.. ”2.02023... .9328 .52 u .5... .22. 2.8.... .o 8.532.. H mm .2 ...—2. $2.... .8 259.... H :m < ...-83.2. 3.22 H 32 m ”own 2.2202 28 u ow< mo 828... .6388 u 2:8... X28» .5 22.828 .3228 H ..m new“ .0288 n.- ow< wU .m:o_.~_>2nn< N 25... .a 32.2... .2. 2.22.28... u <2 .202 on o 2 2 .h mmfi vm o a... mm .N hm o no.3 hm . 2 N v . hm . co. 2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 <2 v o m. . _ ho. . .82 ...:2 Gm x 82nz. m 22,.- 8 o .3 8.. m .... z. o 88 e: o. 2.. an o 2.2 8.8 -.-.< 8.. . 2.. 8.. ... .. 2 c an... 8... .... < 2 o 2 .N SN 32 n. 8.8 ...8 2... 8.8 um... .6 8m... o8.m 2.8.x... 8.28.8 2.8... .6 8 c .3 8.2 ..m. 8 8.8 8.2 8... 8.8 uw< mo 8 8 8.. 8.. 8.5.8... 5.2 5.2 mm x .2? Gmmuz. . as... 8322.3 .922..— 28 2.82 U Nun—22%;. APPENDIX D APPENDIX D UCRIHS Approval Letter mcmcam STATE umvensin' mm m we! "mm“ uni ”MAIN! no lasts!“- 0 mount“ . clue-Mona “I! m n 00 no hum! .m “”00! April 20. 1992 Susan T. Franklin 2066 Hillside Dr. Huskegon. Kl L9éél RE: THE lNFLUlZXCE OF ACQUIRING THE FAMILY ELDER CARE ROLE 0!! FE‘MLE E‘iPlDYHENT ADA?757103. 223 #92-157 Dear 3s. franklin: I an pleased to advise that because of the nature of the proposed research. it was eligible for expedited review. This process has been completed. the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately protected. and your project is therefore approved. You are reninded that UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year. If you plan to continue this project beyond one year. please nake provisions for obtaining appropriate UCRIHS approval prior to April 15. 1993. Any changes in procedures involving human subjects trust be reviewed by the UCRIHS prior to initiat.on of the change. UCRIRS nus: also be notified promptly of any problens (unexpected side effeCts. complaints. etc.) involving human subjects during the course of the work. Thank you for bringing this project to our attention. If we can‘be of any future help. please do not hesitrte to let us know. 1.142% David E. wright. 3h. .. Chair University Commit. - on Research Involving Huzan Subjects (UCRIHS) Sincerely, cc: Dr. Sarbara Ames V \l u a 2"vun r A. to». ! .‘aa' 'kffls om inst-Ian .- 206 ‘Mulloujuflmwoo“