It o . :13 t ”.2. .1; Iii} 1 iv. O. :53: 21,.5633... 9 on L . Ia {Irr- .5..: . . 1.. priivzv.‘ r. . ..c:):.¢.r.492 u' 1...}.ng; .QO.110‘_0;11 15...... r I uklzrl‘rtl. 15.....117’1,‘ . 1!. Ila !’tc¢.| Viol). :3, to!!! vent] . \lfltr v 1 3,1. 175,... "ENE” TTEWVE ”Willi/luluWWW! 19300 110 This is to certify that the thesis entitled RELATIONSHIPS AMONG PERCEPTIONS OF PERSONAL AND FAMILY FUNCTIONING, DEFENSIVE FUNCTIONING, AND WORKING MODELS OF CAREGIVING presented by Steven Alan Meyers has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Mastergof Arts Psychology degree in 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution i’ i LIIRAIIY chhlgu ltetc Unlveulty K A -— u’r- ' w—nc-c PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. l DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE E: C—i—F MSU is An Atflrmetive Action/Equal Opportunity Institution cm“.- RELATIONSHIPS AMONG PERCEPTIONS OF PERSONAL AND FAMILY FUNCTIONING, DEFENSIVE FUNCI‘IONING, AND WORKING MODELS OF CAREGIVING By Steven Alan Meyers A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1992 ABSTRACT RELATIONSHIPS AMONG PERCEPTIONS OF PERSONAL AND FAMILY FUNCTIONING, DEFENSIVE FUN CI'IONING, AND WORKING MODELS OF CAREGIVING By Steven Alan Meyers This study was designed to examine the relationship among self- perceptions, perceptions of family functioning, defensive functioning, and caregiving schemata in a sample of 618 college undergraduates. It was hypothesized that mental representations of personal characteristics (e.g., perceptions of an agentic versus a neurotic self) and representations of family functioning (e.g., perceptions of a psychologically healthy versus unhealthy family) would be systematically associated with the characteristics of respondents’ ”working model" of caregiving. It was further hypothesized that this relationship would be affected by defensive operations which allow the individual to regulate anxiety. Although results indicated that perceptions of personal functioning, perceptions of family functioning, and defensive functioning are systematically related, no consistent evidence was found to support the hypothesis that perceptions of personal and family system characteristics relate to aspects of respondents' ”working models" of caregiving assessed via subject responses to a series of hypothetical parent- child problem situations. Limitations of sample and methodology are discussed, and directions for future research are suggested. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my sincere appreciation to those people who helped in the completion of this project. I am most indebted to the chairperson of my committee, Dr. Gary Stollak. His research was the impetus for this thesis, and he provided me with continuous support and assistance from the start to the finish. Gary has been influential in my development not only as a researcher, but also as a teacher, clinician, and an individual. I am indebted to Dr. Larry Messé and Dr. Joel Aronoff, who served as committee members. I am grateful for Larry’s eloquence in explaining statistical procedures and his commitment to my understanding. I am also thankful for Joel’s interest in this project, his patience, and assistance in conceptualizing the ideas behind this study. I appreciate the efforts of fifteen undergraduates who assisted with data collection, questionnaire coding, and clerical responsibilities. Finally, I thank my family and friends for providing me with support that I needed. In particular, I am indebted to John Loraas for his help with computer analyses, and Mollie Poorman for her understanding and caring. Lastly, I thank Elaine Allensworth for the love, friendship, and warmth that she shares with me. Her continual support makes many stressful times much, much brighter. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Characteristics of the sensitive parent Origins of sensitive parenting Impact of family perceptions on caregiving working models and behavior Impact of personal perceptions on caregiving working models and behavior Defensive functioning and caregiving working models and behavior Hypotheses METHOD Subjects Procedure Measures RESULTS Initial analyses Analyses of PPFC data Analyses of DMI data Analyses of ST C data Refined analyses i v vi 10 11 13 14 14 14 14 19 19 19 iii 27 Relationships between constructs DISCUSSION APPENDICES A. Sensitivity to Children (STC) Questionnaire B Sensitivity to Children (ST C) Coding Manual C. Defense Mechanism Inventory (DMI) Questionnaire D Perceptions of Personal and Family Characteristics (PPFC) Questionnaire E. PPFC Cluster Typology LIST OF REFERENCES 27 E 74 79 Table 1. LIST OF TABLES Factor analysis of the PPFC PPFC factor intercorrelations Factor analysis of ST C: 9 factor solution ST C factor intercorrelations Correlations between PPFC, DMI, and ST C factors Stepwise multiple regression analyses: Prediction of DMI composite scales from PPFC Stepwise multiple regression analyses: Prediction of ST C scales from DMI and PPFC Page 20 25 28 31 31 The goal of understanding the causes and concomitants of future sensitive and insensitive parenting is intriguing from both a clinical and research perspective. It would be of great utility to be able to discriminate between those individuals who are ”at ris ” for later non-optimal caregiving and those who most likely will be sensitive, effective caregivers. As cognitions guide parenting behavior and are predictive of child outcomes (McGillicuddy-Di Lisi, 1985), the assessment of an individual’s ”working model" of caregiving may provide insight into his/ her later effectiveness as a parent. Consequently, the study of the cognitive antecedents of these “working models" of caregiving might be a fruitful endeavor. This work addressed these topics and explored possible relationships among undergraduates’ reports of their personal characteristics and family functioning, defensive functioning, and their responses to children in hypothetical problem situations. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE a eris 'cs 0 esensitive ent One popular line of research and theorizing in developmental and clinical psychology has been the delineation of parenting behaviors or ”styles" that are associated with rearing children who are labeled by teachers and others as ”competent” in terms of personal and social behaviors. For example, in an effort to determine which caregiving behaviors were related to social responsibility, achievement orientation, vitality, and independence in children, Baumrind (197 3, 1989) assessed a sample of nursery-school aged 2 children along the dimensions of instrumental competence, and coded parent-child interaction behaviors in terms of parental control, maturity demands, clarity of communication, nurturance, warmth, and involvement. Three composites, or caregiving ”styles," emerged from her analyses. One style which was characterized by the coupling of firm control with warmth, nurturance and sensitivity in parent-child interactions, the active enforcement of limits, demands for mature behavior; the encouragement verbal give and take, the clear statement of values, and the sharing of reasoning behind parental policies was labeled “authoritative." Children of ”authoritative" parents, as compared to children whose parents were either ”authoritarian” or ”permissive" (see below) were the most self-reliant, competent, and affiliative in Baumrind’s samples. Another parental style which emphasized obedience as a virtue, utilized punitive, forceful measures to obtain compliance with absolute standards, did not encourage child independence, and was characterized by a lack of responsiveness in parent-child interactions was labeled ”authoritarian." Boys from ”authoritarian” households were found to be relatively hostile, while girls were lacking independence and dominance (Baumrind, 1989). The third style which consisted of parental behavior that was somewhat warm but did not exert control over children’s behavior, which made the fewest maturity demands of children, and was nonpunitive and accepting was labeled ”permissive." Compared to children of “authoritative" and ”authoritarian" parents, children of ”permissive" parents were found to score lowest on measures of self-control and self-reliance. The communication of sensitivity in parent-child interactions has been further delineated by Stollak and his colleagues (Stollak, 1992; Stollak, 3 Scholom, Kallman, 8t Saturansky, 1973; Wright & Stollak, 1991). He and other theorists (e.g., Gordon, 1970) state that empathic communications entail (a) clearly indicating awareness of the child's feelings; (b) helping the child understand the relationship between his/ her feelings and behavior and the adult’ 8 feelings and behavior; and, (c) helping the child find appropriate outlets for the expressions of his/ her feelings, needs, and wishes. Although research has described characteristics and consequences of optimal parenting, the process by which an individual becomes a sensitive, “authoritative" parent has remained less clear. How does an individual become a sensitive caregiver? What are the origins of warmth, control, and involvement in parent-child interactions and “working models" of parent- child interaction? This section reviews several theoretical paradigms which elucidate the processes by which an individual's ”working model" of caregiving can be influenced by cognitions, including perceptions of personal characteristics, perceptions of family functioning, and defensive functioning. Social Learning Theory. One framework for analyzing the acquisition of caregiving styles is provided by social learning theory, which asserts that any given behavior is obtained through reinforcement principles and/ or observational learning. Such theorists stress that through observing the behavior of others, ”children can acquire novel behaviors, can discover ways to recombine elements in their existing repertoires, and can become aware of the consequences of their behaviors" (Maccoby 8: Martin, 1983). Characteristics of models influence learning, for imitation is most probable if 4 the model is prestigious, nurturant, and skillful relative to the child’s preexisting level of ability. With this profile in mind, it is probable that parents represent powerful and salient models whose interactional styles and caregiving behaviors may be emulated by their children. Thus, parenting styles can be transmitted from one generation to the next via modeling processes. It is probable that recipients of sensitive parenting become sensitive parents. Conversely, Simons, Whitbeck, Conger, and Chyi-In (1991) report that observing ”harsh parenting” provides children with a “script for the parent role that they enact with their own children." After repeated exposure to insensitive parenting, children may acquire insensitive parenting schemas which may be reflexively employed at a later time. Attachment Theory. More ”dynamic” theories of the acquisition of representations of sensitive caregiving which stress emotion, arousal, and the developing self-system are provided by attachment theorists (e.g., Belsky & Pensky, 1988). Influenced by the ethological literature, Bowlby (1982) advanced the idea that behavior is the product of instinctual systems. The infant’ s tendency to both maintain proximity to the caregiver and to explore the environment serve as the basis for categorizing attachment security (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). Based on his/ her relationship history, the child develops expectations concerning the self and others termed the ”internal working model" (Bowlby, 1982). Thus, mental representations originating from infant-caregiver interaction serve as the basis for the formation of relationship schemata from which the child brings forward a specific organization of ”feelings, needs, attitudes, expectations, cognitions, and behaviors" (Sroufe 6r Fleeson, 1986). 5 This has direct implications for future relationships, as schemata affect the way in which relationship-relevant information is perceived, comprehended, remembered, and learned throughout childhood and adulthood (Rumelhart, 1980). Moreover, these templates contain internalized representations of relationships as wholes, as such the roles of both caregiver and “care—receiver" are retained by the child (Sroufe 6t Fleeson, 1986, 1988). Empirical support of attachment theory. As the manner in which the child relates to others is assumed to be consonant with early attachment experience, researchers have attempted to empirically substantiate the degree to which interactional styles generalize. Sroufe, Fox, and Pancake (1983) found that children from an urban poor sample classified at 12 and 18 months as anxiously attached were overly dependent on preschool teachers at 52 months, based on teacher ratings and behavioral observations. Similarly, Sroufe and Fleeson (1988) report that the relationships of children who as infants were securely attached to their mothers were ”warm, mutually respectful, agreeable, ageoappropriate, and matter of fact" with their teachers. Meanwhile teachers characterized children who as infants were avoidantly attached to mother as noncompliant and needing control. Secure attachment in infancy is similarly related to positive peer relations. These children were rated as socially competent, popular; and highly involved with age-mates (Sroufe 8: Fleeson, 1988). Similarly, Sroufe, Egeland, and Kreutzer (1990) report that those children identified as securely attached at age one-year are considered well liked by peers and possessed clearly identifiable mutual friends at age six. Conversely, avoidant attachment has been associated with frequent hostility, unprovoked 6 aggression, and generally negative peer interactions (LaFreniere 8r Sroufe, 1985). Moreover; attachment quality is related to sociability with unfamiliar; friendlyzstrangers. Twelve-month-old infants who were securely attached to mother were more likely to engage in positive eye contact, respond in an affectively positive fashion, and interact/ engage emotionally and physically with an adult clown than were insecurely attached infants (Main 6: Weston, 1981). To summarize, those individuals who were the recipients of sensitive caregiving during early childhood are more likely to be categorized as “securely attached" and have many psychosocial and psychoeducational skills in their early school years. Specifically, these individuals may perceive themselves as worthy and potent, perceive others as dependable, and are likely to possess the ability to establish healthy, supportive relationships in the spirit of their past relationships. Those individuals who received insensitive parenting (without substitute sensitive caregiving from others) may suffer from broad impairments that affect the development of social skills in childhood and future caregiving abilities: they may perceive themselves as unworthy of care, perceive others as untrustworthy, and consequently have a great deal of difficulty in establishing healthy, supportive relationships. Adult attachment: Retrospective analysis. Instead of assessing parent- child interactions and longitudinally following the sample to observe later parenting, an alternative approach is to assess retrospectively an adult’ 8 perceptions of characteristics of his / her family of origin and to observe 7 his/ her present functioning as a caregiver. The Adult Attachment Interview, used by Mary Main and her colleagues (Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Main, Kaplan, and Cassidy, 1985), asks respondents to describe close relationships, supportive memories, contradictory memories, and assessments of relationships in childhood, and current assessments of the same experiences and relationships. Respondents are further asked whether their parents threatened separation, and whether they ever had felt rejected during childhood. Interviews were rated with respect to (a) the extent to which adults value attachment relationships, and regard experiences related to attachment as influential, (b) the ease in discussing attachment relationships, and (c) the readiness to recall attachment related information. Main, Kaplan, 82 Cassidy (1985) report that those mothers who valued attachment relationships and readily recalled attachment-relevant information were sensitive caregivers with their own children. Conversely, those mothers who perceived that they were rejected during childhood appeared to defensively distort, idealize, and repress information during the interview, and tended to reject their own infants in observed interactions. Using similar, but not identical, criteria to judge mothers' representations of received caregiving, Grossmann, et al. (1988) derived four classifications: a positive representation (characterized by the presence of at least one supportive caregiver; the ability to reflect about childhood experiences, realistic views of parents, and the ability to focus on the topic of attachment), a non-defensive representation (characterized by openness and thoughtful descriptions of early relationships, however individuals lacked a clearly supportive parent), an idealizing, incoherent, or intellectualizing 8 representation, and a repressive representation (indicated by the individual’s inability, recalcitrance, or detachment in remembering early childhood experience). They report strong connections between positive maternal relationship representation and mother-infant interactions marked by sensitivity, understanding, cooperation, and child-centeredness when infants were 2, 6, and 10 months old. - act of f- ' o-ce'o on .15? ' :worc'i- mnels. . o-havm One important determinant of ”working models" of caregiving behavior suggested by theorizing and research is the individual's perception of his/ her family functioning. Thus, the impact of family variables on an individual’s later caregiving is ”mediated both by his childhood and by his adult perceptions of his parents’ behaviors and attitudes" (Bronson, Katten, 8r Livson, 1959). Specifically, it has been argued that an individual's perception of a phenomenon is the most important variable affecting his/ her own behavior (Gecas 6r Schwalbe, 1986). Sroufe and his colleagues advance that mental representations of family interactions determine expectations for future relationships (Sroufe, 1988; Sroufe Gr Fleeson, 1986). Similarly, mothers' recollections of relationships with their mothers (i.e., their children’s maternal grandmothers) were predictive of current caregiving behaviors in retrospective studies of attachment (Grossmann, et al., 1988; Main, Kaplan, 81: Cassidy, 1985). Conceptualizations of perceptions of family functioning. Researchers have demonstrated that perceptions of family health and family environment correlate with individual characteristics which may have an impact on later caregiving behavior (Billings 8: Moos, 1982). One 9 conceptualization of the major dimensions of family functioning has been advanced by Olson and his colleagues (e.g., Olson, 1986; Olson, Russell, 8: Sprenkle, 1983; Olson, Sprenkle, 8: Russell, 1979) who view perceptions of family functioning in terms of two orthogonal variables, family cohesion and family adaptability. They view family cohesion as ”the emotional bonding members have with one another and the degree of individual autonomy a person experiences in the family system." Perceptions of family cohesion range from low (”disengaged families") to high (“enmeshed families”). The second dimension that Olson describes is family adaptability, which refers to ”the ability of a marital / family system to change its power structure, role relationships, and relationship rules in response to situational and developmental stress." Perceptions of family adaptability similarly range from low (”rigid families") to high (”chaotic families"). Although perceptions of moderate levels of family cohesion and moderate levels of family adaptability are hypothesized to be most adaptive and have been associated with optimal intrapsychic and interpersonal functioning (Olson, 1985; Olson, Sprenkle, 8: Russell, 1979), a linear relationship for family adaptability has been documented as well (Green, Harris, Forte, 8: Robinson, 1991; Perosa 8: Perosa, 1990). Consistent relationships have been reported between perceptions of parental availability, trust, communication, and connectedness, and adolescent social competence, self esteem, and emotional adjustment (Armsden 8: Greenberg, 1987; Greenberg, Siege], 8: Leitch, 1983; Rice, 1990). Similarly, Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg, and Dornbusch (1991) report that adolescents (aged 14-18) who characterize their parents as “authoritative" appear self-confident, academically oriented, and socially adapted, while 10 those adolescents who perceive their parents as ”neglectful” display lower levels of competence and higher levels of misbehavior and psychological distress compared to their peers. 111- ct o ore-1m a1 1‘ '9 '0 <1 119:4 ’. wor <1: - .. ls ano be . '