ABSTRACT THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND ANIMAL-ANIMAL INTERACTION IN THE SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION OF GALLERIA MELLONELLA (LEPIDOPTERA) LARVAE BY Peter Edward Hogan The studies in this dissertation deal with the preference behavior of individual Galleria mellonella (L.) larvae for homotypically conditioned food, its relationship to spatial distribution, and the effects of degree of conditioning, development (age), and the presence of conspecifics on these preferences. A mechanism is postulated for the interaction of these variables with larval activity rhythms in determining spatial patterns. Also presented is an analytical tech- nique for the analysis of multiple samples taken from the same indi- viduals over time. In Chapter I is presented a review of the literature on spatial distribution as it specifically relates to invertebrates with an emphasis on insects and the role of biological conditioning in spatial distribution. The effects of animal-animal interactions on preferences for conditioned food are discussed. The studies in this review are population studies in which individual behavior, in respect to homotypic conditioning and animal-animal interaction, has not been thoroughly investigated. Peter Edward Hogan Chapter II presents many life history and behavioral consider- ations directly pertaining to Galleria mellonella larvae, the experi- mental organism for these studies. Chapter III presents the statistical procedures and the under— lying assumptions for multivariate analysis of longitudinal data. Hotelling's One and Two Sample T2 Statistics for Multivariate Analysis are outlined, illustrating a procedure for treating time as a multi- dimensional variate. Emphasized are procedures for testing the assumptions of homogeneous variances and non-serially correlated data. The major objective is to arm behavioral biologists with the necessary analytical techniques enabling logical decisions about tests of significance on repeated individual measurements over time. Chapter IV tests the hypothesis that preference of individual Galleria larvae for homotypically conditioned food is a function of the degree of conditioning and age of the larvae. Larvae were given a two-choice situation in which one food lump was variously conditioned and the other was non-conditioned. Individual preference was recorded every 12 hours for the total developmental period. Depending on the degree of conditioning, larvae are attracted to conditioned food with a threshold effect being indicated at high levels of conditioning. Older larvae demonstrate stronger, initial, attraction to conditioning than do young larvae, although there is some confounding with early experience factors, but initial preference is not a good indicator for long-term preference. There are indications that age differences in preference may be related to differential activity. Adult Galleria, on the other hand, avoid highly conditioned food, avoidance being Peter Edward Hogan measured by oviposition preferences. The consequences of this behavior are discussed in relation to larval density. Chapter V treats the hypothesis that individual larval pref- erences for conditioned food are a function of the presence of con- specifics. A resident larva (always an older larva) in a conditioned food lump has no effect on individual preference for that food lump. However, if two larvae encounter each other in the open, that is when neither larva is in a food lump, both preferences for conditioned food are lowered, as measured by comparisons with an isolate situation. The preference of the younger larva is most affected initially but soon increases, possibly as a function of an interaction with the older larva. A hypothesis was proposed and tested that the behavior of the younger larva is a function of the initial encounter between the two larvae, extra conditioning performed by the older larva on the already conditioned food lump, and a second encounter later in develop- ment. The effect of the initial encounter is supported by the data, but the effects of the other two variables remain inconclusive. Future tests are proposed. In Chapter VI are summarized the results of these studies. Individual behavior and distribution of Galleria larvae is a function of the degree of homotypic conditioning, age of the individual larva, presence and age of a conspecific, location of the conspecific, and possibly activity differences between larvae. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND ANIMAL-ANIMAL INTERACTION IN THE SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION OF GALLERIA MELLONELLA (LEPIDOPTERA) LARVAE BY Peter Edward Hogan A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Zoology 1975 I dedicate this dissertation to my wife, Peggy, whose incalculable patience, understanding, and help have made all phases of this study possible. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my appreciation to Dr. Martin Balaban, my advisor, for his help and encouragement throughout my graduate career and for his critical evaluation of my research and this dissertation. I am also grateful to the members of my guidance committee including Dr. John A. King, Dr. James H. Asher, and Dr. Stanley C. Ratner for their constructive comments during this research. Special thanks go to Dr. James H. Asher for the time spent helping me with the mathematical analyses in this dissertation and to Lincoln Gray for his invaluable help in writing the computer pro- grams used in the analyses. Without their help this dissertation would not have been possible. This research was supported in part by NIH Animal Behavior Training Grant #5 T01 GMOl751-OS BHS and by teaching assistantships provided by the Department of Zoology. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . LIST OF FIGURES . CHAPTER I. BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION . Introduction . Literature Review Spatial Distribution . Sense Organs . Food and Spatial Distribution Environmental Factors in Spatial Distribution Sex and Genetic Factors in Spatial Distribution . Density Factors in Spatial Distribution Biological Conditioning, Defined . Physiological Aspects of Biological Conditioning . Protection From Toxic Substances . Control of Sex . Effects on Morphological Changes . Effects on Growth Effects on Population Dynamics . Biological Conditioning and Spatial Distribution . iv Page ix XV 11 16 19 CHAPTER II. III. IV. BEHAVIORAL AND LIFE HISTORY CONSIDERATIONS OF THE GREATER WAX MOTH, GALLERIA MELLONELLA (L.) (LEPIDOPTERA: GALLERIIDAE) . . . . Behavior and Life History Spinning Behavior and Preferences Food Preferences . Rearing and Maintenance of Stock Cultures HOTELLING'S T2 STATISTIC FOR MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS: A "NEW" APPROACH TO LONGITUDINAL STUDIES AND SERIALLY CORRELATED DATA . Introduction . Hotelling's One Sample T2 Statistic Hotelling's Two Sample T2 Statistic The Assumptions Underlying Hotelling's Procedure . Multivariate Normal Distribution . Equality of Variance-Covariance Matrices . Serially Uncorrelated Data . Summary BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION IN GALLERIA MELLONELLA (L.) LARVAE Introduction . Materials and Methods Husbandry Apparatus Experimental Procedures Analysis . Pilot Experiment . Data Reduction . Page 65 65 71 75 78 83 83 86 93 99 100 102 105 107 108 108 110 110 111 111 117 127 130 CHAPTER Page Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 I. Within-Group Comparisons for Randomness of Larval Response to Conditioning . . . . . . . . . 131 II. Between-Group Comparisons of Larval Responses to Increasing Degrees of Conditioning . . . . . . 134 A. Response of Test Larvae to Conditioning Done By Varying the Number of Days of Conditioning Within Any One Age of Conditioning Larva . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 B. Response of Test Larvae to Conditioning Done By Varying Ages of Conditioning Larvae at Constant Days of Conditioning . . . . . . . 147 III. Between-Group Comparisons of the Effects of Larval Age on Response to Conditioning . . . . . . 158 IV. A Brief Analysis Relating to Initial Pref- erence to Conditioned Food and the Amount of Larval Movement as Conditioning Increases . . . . 170 A. Does Larval Initial Preference to Con- ditioned Food Change as Degree of Con- ditioning Increases . . . . . . . . . . . . . 170 B. Does Amount of Larval Movement Vary in Different Amounts of Conditioning? . . . . . . 179 Control for Food Limitation . . . . . . . . . . . . 181 V. Can Response to Conditioning Be Overridden by Varying the Food Source? . . . . . . . . . 182 VI. Do Adult Galleria Exhibit Preferences to Conditioning Done by Larvae? . . . . . . . . . 185 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18S Larval Responses to Increasing Degrees of Conditioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 187 Age Differences in Response to Conditioning and Possible Mechanisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188 Size of Responding Larvae . . . . . . . . . . . 188 Activity Differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 189 vi CHAPTER Sensory Differences Between Ages . Early Experience Factors . Response to Biological Conditioning as a Bioassay of Behavior . Possible Functions of Biological Conditioning Protection . Biological Conditioning as a Preferred Consistency Mobility . A Signal for the Presence of Other Larvae Biological Conditioning and Feeding Behavior . Biological Conditioning and Population Density . V. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND ANIMAL-ANIMAL INTERACTION IN THE SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION OF GALLERIA MELLONELLA (L.) LARVAE . Introduction . Materials and Methods Husbandry Apparatus Experimental Procedures Analysis . Results Experiment I: Do Galleria Larvae Group in the Absence of Initial Conditioning and Is Their Distribution in Such a Situation a Function of the Presence of a Conspecific? . . . . . . Experiment 11: When Initial Conditioning is Present, Is Preference for Conditioning Affected by the Presence of a Conspecific, the Age of the Conspecific, and Whether ‘the Conspecific is a Resident in the Conditioned Food? vii Page 191 192 193 196 196 197 197 198 198 200 201 201 204 204 204 204 208 209 231 (CHAPTER Experiment III: Are the Lowered Responses of Younger, 7 Day Old Larvae, When a 13 Day Old Larva is Simultaneously Introduced, a Result of the Changing Biological Conditioning, Interaction With the 13 Day Old Larva, or Both? . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . Individual Responses to a Conspecific and Biological Conditioning Possible Mechanisms for the Interaction of Biological Conditioning and Larval-Larval Interaction in the Spatial Distribution of Galleria Larvae . VI. SUMMARY Isolate Behavior . Interactions Between Conspecifics and Biological Conditioning . BIBLIOGRAPHY viii Page 255 274 276 281 290 290 292 295 Table 3.1. 4.2. 4.3. 4.4. 4.5. LIST OF TABLES Artificial dietary medium for laboratory rearing of the Greater Wax Moth, Galleria mellonella (L.) Components of the Two Sample T2 Statistic Larval weights corresponding to the larvae in Figure 4.1. Weights were calculated for 500 randomly selected larvae of a particular age as well as for the larvae of a particular age actually used for experimentation Twelve experimental groups in Figure 4.2 were randomly selected and their AM and PM mean vectors were compared using Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample Analyses . . . . . Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of each group from Figure 4.2 with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5 to determine which groups are random. Mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (82), F-values, and D-values for serial correlation are given Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the response of 7 day old test larvae to varying amounts of conditioning (1,3, 5, and 7 days) done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the response of 11 day old test larvae to varying amounts of conditioning (1, 3, 5, and 7 days) done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae . . . . . . . . . . . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the response of 13 day old test larvae to varying amounts of conditioning (1,3, 5, and 7 days) done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix Page 80 95 112 118 123 136 139 142 Table 4.7. 4.9. 4.10. 4.11. 4.12. 4.13. 4.14. Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the response of 7 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held con- stant at l, 3, 5, and 7 days . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the response of 11 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held con- stant at l, 3, 5, and 7 days . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the response of 13 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held con- stant at l, 3, 5, or 7 days Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons for age differences in response to conditioning. The responses of test larvae (7, 11, and 13 days old) to l, 3, S, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 6 day old larva are tested . . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons for age differences in response to conditioning. The responses of test larvae (7, 11, and 13 days old) to l, 3, S, or 7 days of conditioning by a 9 day old larva are tested . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons for age differences in response to conditioning. The responses of test larvae (7, 11, and 13 days old) to l, 3, 5, and 7 days of conditioning by a 12 day Old larva are tested Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample tests on comparable 4 day segments of the response vectors shown to exhibit age differ- ences by a Two-Sample comparison in Tables 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12 . Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of randomly selected experimental groups from Figure 4.2 with their replications as a test of whether behavior is changing over time and from hatch to hatch Page 148 151 154 159 162 165 169 172 Table 4.15. 4.16. 4.17. 4.18. 4.19. 4.20. 5.1. 5.2. 5.3. Chi-square analysis of initial larval preference to the extremes of conditioning within the three test ages . Results of a Chi-square goodness of fit analysis on the number of times larvae moved between conditioned and plain food lumps when conditioning is either low or high . . . Analysis of variance on developmental times of 7, 11, and 13 day old larvae reared on single food lumps which were either maximally conditioned or non-conditioned . Analysis of variance on 11 day old larvae reared on normal and deficient diets . . . . A 2 x 2 contingency table analyzed by chi-square of larval responses to normal-conditioned and deficient-conditioned food . Chi-square goodness of fit test on female adult preference for egg-laying in low and high larval conditioned food Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of experimental groups (P) versus the replications (R) from Figure 5.1. Com- parisons are made on the basis of total number of pairs, regardless of which food lump they were in Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of each experimental (P) group in Figure 5.1 with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5 to see if grouping (pairing), regardless of food lump, is different from random in the absence of homgtypic conditioning. Response mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (8 ), and D-values for serial correlation are found in Table 5.1 . Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of pairs, regardless of food lump, versus an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. These comparisons are shown for the first four days and the last four days of each experimental group to see if time in the experimental situation affects grouping behavior . xi Page 178 180 182 183 184 186 213 218 219 Table Page 5.4. Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons among the experimental groups in Figure 5.1. Comparisons are made for the first 4 days and the last 4 days to see if time in the experimental situation affects grouping, and all comparisons are for pairs of larvae regardless of food lump. For the One Sample (') Test the groups used for the expected vectors are in the column headed EV. and those whose covariance matrix was used are in the column headed CV . . . . . . 221 5.5. Contingency tables with Chi-square Analysis for each group in Figure 5.1 with the total Chi- square partitioned to show the contribution of each time point for each food lump to the total. Within each box are shown the total number of pairs of larvae for each food lump at each time point and their individual Chi-square values. Also shown are the total pairs at each time point or food lump and their Chi-square contribution and the total Chi-square value over all time points and both food lumps . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 224 5.6. Results of Hotelling's One Sample analysis on the preference of the younger larva of a pair for the food lump in which the older larva initially selected and remained in for the entire experi- mental period. Comparison is made against an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5 . . . . . . . 227 5.7. Analysis of Variance on developmental times of two 7, two 11, and two 13 day old larvae reared on single conditioned or plain food lumps . . . . . . . . . 230 5.8. This table presents the response mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (82), and D-values for serial correlation for the groups in Figures 5.3 and 5.4 to be used in future comparisons . . . . . . . . 234 5.9. Hotelling's One Sample test on the experimental groups in Figures 5.4 and 5.5. Total larvae in the con- ditioned food (paired, singly, or none) is compared with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. Response mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (82), and D-values for serial correlation are found in Table 5.8 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 236 xii Table 5.10. Hotelling's One Sample test on the experimental groups in Figures 5.4 and 5.5. Pairs of larvae on conditioned food are compared with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.25. Response mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (S ), and D-values for serial corre- lation are found in Table 5.8 5.11. Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons among the groups in Figure 5.4. Assuming no animal—animal interaction, the response vectors for two larvae of equal age should be the same as the response of one larva of the same age. Also the response vector for either larvae, when the two test larvae are unequal in age, should be the same as the response of one larvae of the appropriate age 5.12. Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons among the experimental groups in Figures 5.4 and 5.5 to determine resident versus non-resident effects on preference for biological conditioning. Assuming no animal-animal interactions there should not be any differences in these comparisons. There should also not be any differences if the effect of a resident larva on the response larva is the same as the effect of another larva introduced simul- taneously with the response larva 5.13. This table presents the response mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors ($2), and D-values for serial correlation for the groups to be analyzed in Table 5.14 5.14. Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of the 7 day old mean response vectors when paired with an 11, 13, or 15 day old larva or when an isolate, all with initial high conditioning on one food lump 5.15. Hotelling's One Sample (') comparison of the first 3 days of the 7 day old response vectors when with various aged conspecifics and when isolated, with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5 . xiii Page 238 240 248 258 261 263 Table 5.16. 5.17. 5.18. 5.19. 5.20. Table of mean response vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (52), and D-values for serial correlation of the 7 day old larvae from the experimental groups being tested for the effects of initial encounter, extra conditioning, and the second encounter with a 13 day old con- specific . Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons among the 7 day old response vectors in Table 5.16 Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons among the 7 day old response vectors in Table 5.16 for the last 3 days of each mean response vector . . . . . . Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of the first 5 data points of each group from Table 5.16 with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5 Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample comparisons of various groups replicated through- out this chapter . xiv Page 266 267 271 273 275 LIST OF FIGURES Hotelling's One Sample T2 Statistic Hotelling's Two Sample T2 Statistic From right to left, these are 6, 7, 9, ll, 12, 13, and 15 day old larvae used for conditioning and testing preference for conditioning. Note the size distribution of the larvae Experimental design, showing the identification labels and the date when each experimental (B) group and control (C) group was run Pilot experiment to see if Galleria larvae response to biologically conditioned food. Note that after 16 days of age the response begins declining during which time the larvae are apparently not exhibiting a preference for either conditioned or non-conditioned food Mean response vectors of 7 day old test larvae to varying amounts of conditioning (1, 3, 5, and 7 days) done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae Mean response vectors of 11 day old test larvae to varying amounts of conditioning (l, 3, 5, and 7 days) done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae Mean response vectors of 13 day old test larvae to varying amounts of conditioning (l, 3, 5, and 7 days) done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae Mean response vectors of 7 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held constant at 1, 3, 5, or 7 days Mean response vectors of 11 day Old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held constant at l, 3, 5, or 7 days XV Page 87 94 112 113 128 138 141 144 150 153 Figure 4.9. Mean response vectors of 13 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held constant at l, 3, 5, or 7 days . Mean response vectors of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to l, 3, 5, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 6 day old larva . . Mean response vectors of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to l, 3, 5, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 9 day old larva . . Mean response vectors of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to l, 3, 5, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 12 day old test larva . . Replications of randomly selected groups from the experimental design in Figure 4.2 Experimental design for asking whether larvae will group (pair) in the absence of homotypic conditioning . Mean response vectors for percent pairs of larvae on any food lump when the food lumps were initially unconditioned Mean response vectors of the younger larvae in a pair to a particular "plain" food lump that was initially selected by the older member of the pair and in which the older larva remained in for the entire experimental period. That food lump is considered the "conditioned" lump for purposes of these curves . Experimental design (top) for animal-animal inter- actions, at two levels of conditioning, when the conditioning larva is removed and both test larvae are new to the experimental situation Experimental design (left) for the effect of a resi- dent larva on the preference of a test larva for biological conditioning. The resident larva is the original conditioning larva xvi Page 156 161 164 167 171 211 217 228 Figure 5.6. 5.7. 5.8. 5.9. 5.10. 5.11. 5.12. Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.11) of two larvae situations with one larva situ- ations to find if individual preference for low conditioned food is affected by the presence of a conspecific . Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.11) of two larvae situations with one larva situations to find if individual preference for high con- ditioned food is affected by the presence of a conspecific . . . . . . . . . Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.12) of resident versus non-resident effects on larval preference for low conditioning Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.12) of resident versus non-resident effects on larval preference for high conditioning . Mean response vectors of a 7 day old larva and a 13 day old larva with the wandering phase of the 13 day old larva included. These are from experi- mental group 64PH(l-5)7+13 . Mean response vectors of 7 day old larvae to high conditioning when a 15, 13, or 11 day old con- specific is also present. The mean response vectors for the 15, 13, and 11 day old larvae are shown, with their wandering phases, for comparative purposes . Response mean vectors for the 7 day old larvae from all experimental groups in Table 5.16 and 5.17 . xvii Page 243 246 251 253 256 260 269 CHAPTER I BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION Introduction Biological conditioning has been defined (Allee 1934) as ". . changes produced in the medium by organisms living therein and the effects of such changes upon other organisms . . . ." Changes in the medium may be chemical or physical and either of homotypic or heterotypic origin. Homotypic conditioning is produced by a con- specific, whereas conditioning emanating from other than a conspecific is referred to as heterotypic. As organisms inhabit their environment they modify it, often to such an extent that whole populations are affected. With invertebrates, notably Lepidoptera, a few studies have related biological conditioning to spatial patterns, although most of the literature treats conditioning in relation to its physiological effects upon other organisms and populations. Since biological con- ditioning was considered a population phenomenon, these studies looked at whole populations, making no attempt to ask questions about how biological conditioning affects individual behavior. The research for this dissertation deals with an analysis of spatial distribution (aggregation and/or dispersal) in the wax moth larvae, Galleria mellonella (Lepidoptera), when maintained in the laboratory. In a simplistic sense, aggregation may be viewed as the 1 result of those factors that "pull" organisms together, and dispersal as the result of those factors that "push" them apart. Spatial dis- tribution (dispersion) would be the result of the interactions between these "pulls" and "pushes." Relationships between biological con- ditioning, animal interactions, and development (age) are examined. The basic approach is to analyze spatial distribution by isolating the critical variables in simple systems of individuals or small groups. Concomitant with this biological approach is the development of an analytical model for spatial distribution. Galleria mellonella was chosen for these studies because of its suitability for behavioral, distributional, and populational experi- mentation, and because the larvae produce a pronounced physical (and possibly chemical) conditioning of their environment. Galleria larvae are vagile, asocial, asexual, short-lived and herbivorous. Naylor (1959) pointed out that these features allow the analysis of behavioral and spatial relationships. Vagility is the ability to actively move from place to place. Asociality is here defined as the ability of an individual to survive and/or reproduce in the absence of structured groups. Galleria adults are extremely fecund, enabling generation of large sample sizes for experimentation. The implication of being short-lived is that Galleria larvae must quickly respond to cues. Herbivorous species usually find food and shelter in a relatively fixed location, and Galleria larvae are not normally found ranging over vast expanses of territory for their life needs. The adults lack mouth parts, do not feed, and are not usually found in close association with the larvae. A consequence of the preceding traits is that the behavior of Galleria is a relatively "simple" system for elucidating the critical variables relating to spatial distribution. Several variables present in other systems, such as sexual relationships and adult-larval inter- actions, are not of prime importance. The following general hypotheses were examined in this dis— sertation: l. The spatial distribution of Galleria larvae is a function of their preferences for homotypic conditioning. 2. Preferences for biological conditioning depend upon the degree of conditioning and the age of the responding larvae. 3. Animal-animal interactions (presence of conspecifics) alter preferences for conditioning, and the degree to which prefer- ences are altered is a function of age and prior residence in the conditioned situation. Literature Review §patial Distribution Dispersion is the spatial structure of a population, as opposed t0 dispersal which is the dynamic process involved (Surtees 1963a, 1964C). Lack's (1954) definition is more rigorous. He defines dis- PerSion as a non-random type of distribution, with dispersal being the movement of young animals from their place of birth. Dispersal is normally thought to consist of (l) organisms leaving their natal site, (2) Crossing some sort of barrier, and (3) settling and breeding in a new place. These criteria have strong genetic implications. However, for the purposes of my studies, dispersal is being defined as the process by which an organism or group of organisms move away from some original site, whether or not breeding occurs in the new locality. In this context dispersal is the opposite of aggregation, and is merely one side of a broader concept, dispersion. Wynne-Edwards (1962) defines dispersion as the placement of individuals and groups of indi- viduals within the habitats they occupy, and the processes by which this is brought about. A general definition, such as this, encompasses other than non-random groupings. Dispersion, contagious distributions, aggregations, groups, bunches, clusters, or whatever label one wishes to give such phenom- enon, are Of widespread occurrence in nature, and are found in varying degrees of complexity throughout the animal phyla. They range from chance assemblages to the highly organized social insects and man. Natural aggregations may result from the response of individuals to the presence of conspecifics or from the independent response of each individual to an external stimulus (Bowen 1931), the former often being called social and the latter asocial. Allee (1927), however, noted that once established, aggregations may last for extended periods of time merely due to a lack of disruptive stimuli; whereas dispersal may be the result of a lack of cohesive stimuli. No social instinct need be evoked to account for the formation or maintenance of aggregations. Wynne-Edwards (1962), on the other hand, states that the maintenance of the spatial organization of animal populations can be attributed to behavioral interactions among its members. An implication of this statement is that whatever the causative factors of aggregation are, individuals would exhibit different distributions if other conspecifics were absent. The critical variables being considered in my studies are bio- logical conditioning and animal-animal interaction. Other causative factors in spatial patterns are frequently encountered in the litera- ture, such as the role of the sense organs, food, and various environ- mental cues. In addition, the mobility of organisms is important and this may depend on individual variables such as sexual and genetic dif— ferences. These factors must be either controlled or considered as variables. It is not the intent of this review to discuss each of these at length. However, since such variables can affect preferences for biological conditioning as well as animal-animal interactions, and since they form the framework for much of the spatial distribution literature, they will be briefly discussed. Sense Organs Sense organs serve as the passageway by which all sensations from the environment reach the central nervous system and elicit some kind of response. Auditory, visual, olfactory, tactual, and electrical signals may all function in dispersionary as well as social behavior. (See Wynne-Edwards 1962 for a review of this topic.) Two basic approaches might be used to study the effects of biological conditioning on spatial distribution. One is to find out what constitutes conditioning and which senses are employed in respond- ing to it. The second approach is to demonstrate a response to con- ditioning, regardless of the senses employed or the characteristics of conditioning, and begin asking questions about the results, function, and dynamics of the responses. I have opted for the latter approach in my studies, although the first approach must eventually be studied. To study the relationship between biological conditioning and spatial distribution it is not necessary to know what conditioning consists of or what senses are used to respond. It is, however, necessary to demonstrate that conditioning is occurring, that it emanates from the organism in question, and that the organism responds to it. The degree of conditioning can be controlled in terms of time spent con- ditioning and age of the organism doing the conditioning. Food and Spatial Distribution Most animals, through their power of movement, play a part in determining their own population densities. They are able to concen- trate at places where food and other resources are plentiful and to Javoid unfavorable situations. According to Wynne-Edwards (1962), food is always the critical or limiting resource which dictates how high a population density can be supported in any particular habitat. Regardless of food supply, however, other variables such as animal- animal interactions and biological conditioning play a major role in distribution. Wynne-Edwards (1962), using some of Jesperson's (1924) data noted that a high correlation exists between plankton density and the density of pelagic birds. He concluded that the birds disperse them- selves on the basis of available food supply. A Territoriality, a form of dispersion, in birds may be food limited. Howard (1920) found that territorial size may be reduced in the presence of abundant food, and may increase in size when food > s. becomes limited. The relationship between food, dispersion, and numbers in a population was discussed by Lack (1954) who concluded that many bird species are limited in numbers, and in resultant distribution, by their food supply. Similar data is available for other Species as well. Calhoun (1948, 1952) with rats, and Brown (1953) and Strecker (1954) with house mice, have demonstrated that location of food may be a factor in influencing spatial distribution under semi-natural conditions. Cal- houn's (1950) study also shows that population size and competition are important in determining spatial distribution. However, the results of such experiments indicate that being near food enables elimination of other individuals from the area. This is an indication that animal-animal interaction, not food supply, is the proximal cause for their distribution. Food has often been shown to be a variable in the spatial dis- tribution of invertebrates. Loschiavo (1952) has shown that Tribolium confusum exhibit strong food preferences which in turn affect its dis- tribution. In fact, flour is not the preferred medium of the flour beetle, if a more nutritional medium is available. Galleria mellonella larvae also utilize foods differentially as well as exhibiting food preferences (Chase 1921; Milum and Geuther 1935; Whitcomb 1946; Beck 1960). Crowding, often related to food, may affect an organism's physiology as well as its distribution. In a study on crowding of the larvae of the meal moth, Ephestia kuhniella, Smith (1969) found that larvae infesting food supplies in an uncrowded state grow faster and larger than those in crowded substrates. This is rather a common observation among other species as well, and has been observed in our laboratory with Galleria larvae. However, Smith did not determine if stunted growth was due to crowding, food limitation, or excessive con- ditioning of the medium. Smith also observed that less crowded cul- tures of Ephestia reproduce faster and probably more plentifully. Rose (1959) discovered similar results in guppies, as have Sang (1949) in Drosophila melanogaster, Park (1938) in Tribolium confusum, Brown (1946) in trout fry, and Rose (1960) in tadpoles. Smith postulated that this faster and more plentiful reproduction maximized population size in the next generation, ensuring maximal use of a food supply. However, none of these researchers made any attempt to separate the effects of crowding and food supply from the effects of biological con- ditioning in crowded cultures. Smith (1969) also noted a behavioral response of Ephestia adults in crowded cultures, the adults being more active than isolates. Such activity may promote the colonization of suitable surrounding food supplies as they become available and as the old supply becomes crowded. A behavioral response of organisms to some aspect of their food supply may affect their distribution. Butterfly larvae of Mechantis isthmia avoid the trichomes on their Solanum plant host by spinning a network of silk over the tops of the spines (Rathcke and Poole 1975). The larvae can then crawl over the tops of the spines cn1 their "silken scaffolding" and safely feed on the unprotected edges of the leaves. Most larvae of this genus are solitary feeders, but larvae of Mechantis isthmia generally feed in small groups of from four to six individuals per leaf, spinning and sharing the webbing together. Rathcke and Poole (1975) advanced the hypothesis that such a silken bridgework is energetically feasible only if several larvae pool their resources and share the benefits. The ability to spin silk is widespread among the Lepidopterans, as well as in other orders. Most larvae spin silk to secure themselves while molting or to attach or protect their pupa. Therefore, evolution of a feeding web would entail only an elaboration of an already present ability. However, the increased silk production could necessitate a change in the social behavior from solitary to gregarious. Such an hypothesis might also apply to Galleria larvae if Paddock's hypothesis (1913) that silk tunnels function to hold the damage combs together after feeding is tenable. There are also situations in which food is not limited and the distribution of a population of organisms is a function of how they behaviorally exploit a preferred food supply. Aphids feed on the phloem cells of plants, imbibing soluble nutrients such as sugars and amino acids. Observations by Way and Cammell (1970) indicated that aphids normally aggregate in groups of about twenty, and that a rela- tionship might exist between these aggregations and food utilization. 'They discovered that aggregates of aphids act as "sinks" diverting nutrients from distant plant parts, and that isolates could not as «Effectively feed. However, as these aggregates enlarged, population grownfiuwas halted as a result of decreased natality. Therefore, aphids 10 with spaced out populations, or in small aggregations, do little harm to a plant and maintain relatively stable populations. Way and Cammell further noted that different species exhibited behavioral differences in exploitation of host plants, with a resultant difference in aggre- gation behavior. Those species normally exploiting a host plant piecemeal remained closely aggregated, i.e., the cabbage and bean aphids, whereas those species normally doing little harm to a host plant remain spaced out in tiny aggregations, i.e., peach and potato aphids. There are innumerable examples of the effects of food on spatial patterns, and food is probably the ultimate factor in any long term dispersionary study. The main reason for discussing it, other than elucidating it as an important variable, is that many studies in spatial distribution failed to account for limited food supplies. Several studies in this review failed to do so, and, as such, require cautious interpretation. For example, the results of Surtee's studies (see "Environmental Factors in Spatial Distribution") are interpretable on the basis of upward movement away from used up food supplies, rather than on the basis of Surtee's hypothesis of upward dispersal under high density as a kinesis. Similar problems arise in Park's work with Tribolium, and Park admitted that the food limitation hypothesis needs testing before his physiological and distributional data can be veri- fied. However, all of my studies described in this dissertation con- trolled for limited food source as a dispersionary agent. 11 Environmental Factors in Spatial Distribution It is well documented that organisms respond to environmental cues, such as temperature, light, and various structural parameters of the environment, and that such responses, often associated with physio- logical tolerances, may affect spatial patterns. Selected examples from vertebrates and invertebrates illustrate this point. Studies in habitat selection (Harris 1952; Wecker 1963; Fitch 1974) have demonstrated that mice prefer certain habitats, which may be related to environmental, genetic, and early experience variables. Habitat characteristics, such as fallen trees and holes in the ground, have been shown to affect the spatial distribution of small mammals. Brand (1955, from Terman 1961), using Peromyscus leucopus, showed a direct seasonal relationship between population distribution and tree density, density of fallen trees, and degree of slope of fallen trees. The significant factors in this relationship are probably potential nest sites and food. The number of rats in city blocks was temporarily decreased by removing harborage sites (Orgain and Schein 1953), and Davis (1958) changed the spatial distribution in laboratory populations of house mice by introducing baffles and additional nest sites. Moisture, ground depressions, hard textured logs, plant cover, and high relative humidity all positively influence the movements of the salamander, Plethodon cinereus (Heatwole 1962). Most of these variables are directly related to the salamander's physiological limits. Varying spatial patterns in relation to environmental Stimuli and physiological tolerances have also been documented in the ant Formica (Talbot 1934), termites Reticulitermes hesperus and 12 R, tibialis (Williams 1934), grasshopper Melanoplus (Parker 1930), salamander Plethodon (Shelford 1913), and wireworm populations (Salt and Hollick 1946; Thorpe e£_al. 1946). In some instances such varying distributions are seasonally correlated. Often spatial distribution is not so much a result of environ- mental factors as it is a result of an organism's mobility limits. For example, Bovbjerg (1952) investigated the aggregation behavior of the aquatic snail Campeloma decisum and found that these aggregations were a result of an upstream dispersal, probably a rheotaxis, coupled with an inability to traverse even slight blockages of the streams. Abiotic variables, such as light, temperature, and humidity, are the most commonly investigated variables in spatial distribution. In the spruce budworm (Choristoneura fumiferana), for example, Wellington (1948) demonstrated that young stages aggregated as a result of being photopositive. Older larvae exhibit a reversal in this ori- entation. Phototaxis is also the predominant factor in groupings of the sea urchins Arbacia punctulata and Lythechinus variegatus (Sharp and Gray 1962). Arbacia is negatively phototactic. Lythechinus is positively phototactic to artificial light and negatively phototactic to sunlight. Lythechinus also exhibits an interesting "heaping" 'behavior as a response to light which consists of picking up shells and seaweed and covering its body with them. There is considerable literature on the spatial patterns in iSOpods in relation to abiotic factors. Warburg (1964) compared several isopod species, finding aggregation differences in relation tx: light, temperature, and humidity. Previously Allee (1926) and 1. 13 Cole (1946) had shown that temperature and dryness affect the "bunch- ing" phenomenon in isopods. Warburg, however, tested several species and discovered species as well as mechanism differences in response. In the grain beetle (Sitophilus gganarius), vertical distribution depends upon temperature, humidity, and density of organisms (Surtees 1963a). At low and high temperatures the number of weevils at the surface increases as a function of density. Increased moisture decreases surface aggregations. In a study on the saw-toothed grain beetle (Oryzaephilus surinamensis), Surtees (1963a) discovered the same kind of relationship between density and moisture, but species differences were observed. Species comparisons of vertical aggregation behavior in relation to moisture, temperature, and density were there- fore performed by Surtees (1964e). The general conclusion from these studies is that accumulation of adults occurs under those conditions where kinesis becomes minimal. Reduction of individual kinesis levels has a secondary effect within groups of adults in that it depresses mutual disturbance which would otherwise elicit dispersal. Accumu- lation within the physical conditions of temperature and moisture is therefore affected by changes in orthokinesis and klinokinesis. In all species examined, specific rates of turning and levels of speed ‘under various conditions of temperature, moisture, and density were :found. Aggregation would be expected in regions of lowest velocity and fewest turns. These conditions prevail under the proper con- ciitions of temperature and humidity. Increasing density has the Opposing effect of dispersing the weevils away from such aggregations. However, the effects of biological conditioning and limited food 14 reserves were not controlled in these experiments and both are viable alternative hypotheses to Surtees' explanation for dispersal. Surtees (1964f) further discovered that aggregation behavior is affected by the beetle's early experience with humidity differences. Beetles reared at 70% R.H. did not accumulate in damp grain, whereas those reared at 40% R.H. for 14 days did. Humidity-conditioned adults moved slowly in damp grain, unconditioned adults moved rapidly. These results add further support to his hypothesis that accumulation in one area must be seen as the joint result of a reduction in individual movement and a consequent depression of intra-group disturbance, which would otherwise elicit dispersal. A similar result was found by Graham (1958) with Tribolium confusum and I, castaneum. Both species exhibit temperature preferences which affect their dispersal. Early experience, however, alters these preferences. Dispersion behavior is often interpreted in light of the variable being examined, when in fact some other, possibly uncon- trolled, variable is the actual mechanism. The woodlice literature is a case in point. Woodlice live in places of high moisture content, i.e., beneath bark, under rocks and fallen trees, or beneath leaves, and humidity had been accepted as the controlling variable in their habitat choice and degree of activity. However, since crevices are Idark, as well as damp, phototactic and hygrokinetic behaviors were Ixrobably linked (Edney 1954). It had also been assumed but never ‘tested, by Allee (1926), that woodlice react to contact stimuli, since (:revices are confining and when crevices are not available they climb (”1 each other and form aggregations. Friedlander (1964) attempted to 15 sort out the responses of the woodlice Oniscus ascellus, Porcellio scabes, and Armadillidium vulgare and how they relate to aggregation behavior. His particular interest was with thigmokinetic behavior, or the response to contact. The results indicate that although the effects of thigmokinetic reactions are similar to humidity reactions, they are distinct from them. Such responses also varied with species and degree of humidity, being most marked at low humidity. The thigmo- kinetic responses depend upon area and roughness of the contacted surface, and increases in either results in reduced locomotion. It would appear that woodlice are drawn to crevices by humidity responses, and that if the crevice provides the proper thigmokinetic stimuli, movement is slowed and aggregations form.' However, Fried- lander did not perform experiments on thigmokinetic behavior as a result of touching other individuals so attracted to a crevice. It is reasonable to assume that as the number of individuals increases in a crevice, thigmokinetic responses to these individuals may override the tendency to slower movement and lead to dispersal. This is very much what happens in grain beetle populations, as seen earlier. It is obvious that as environmental factors vary, so does spatial distribution. In all of my experiments, however, light, temperature, and humidity were held constant in order not to be involved in any spatial distribution results with Galleria populations. 'This was necessary for several reasons, one of which is that there armaindications that abiotic variables affect biological conditioning. fhxr example, low temperatures decrease spinning activity and spinning aurtivity may be the main component of conditioning in Galleria larvae. 16 Sex and Genetic Factors in Spatial Distribution The mobility of an organism has important consequences in relation to the ability to aggregate or disperse. Mobility may depend upon the individual variables of sex and genetics. McDonald (1968), using Tribolium confusum, found that males more rapidly disperse than females and that there are strain differences in mobility. Strain differences may be due to differential response to repellent sub- stances released by the organisms or to avoiding other adults. This was not tested. However, he did find that the mobility of males is lessened in the presence of a female. This supports Naylor's (1959) finding that male Tribolium confusum are attracted to females and exhibit a different distribution pattern than if females are absent from the population. Since genetic differences in mobility and dispersal do exist, it would be helpful to measure the amount of genetic variation popu- lations contain for such traits (or at least control them in experi- mentation) and to explore the relationship between mobility and other fitness components such as fertility and fecundity. In Tribolium, certain behavioral traits are easily studied and modifiable by arti- ficial selection. Dawson (1964) found that individuals of highly inbred lines of Tribolium confusum spend a greater percent of time on ‘the surface of the flour medium than the normal wild type. Sokoloff (1966) extended this finding to mutant strains and demonstrated that ‘therresponse was maintained independently of population density, in (nilture containers. The general activity levels of wild type and certain body color mutants were compared by McDonald and Fitting (1965) 17 who found the latter to be more active. Using Tribolium confusum and I, castaneum, Lerner and Inouze (1968) were able to achieve rapid response to selection for speed of running hierarchical T-mazes. Artificial selection has demonstrated that mobility may be altered, but the exact mechanism is as yet unclear. It is possible that what is being selected for is response to some cue rather than increased mobility. Ogden (1969) experimented with some olfactory cues which mediate dispersal behavior in Tribolium. He attempted (1970b) to determine dispersal behavior, defined as emigratory movement by walking in a laboratory apparatus. The apparatus utilized was that originally designed by Prus (1963), consisting of two shell vials connected with a U-tube through which a thread was strung. The appa- ratus was so designed as to allow only one-way movement through the tube. Ogden tested fourth generation beetles as to their rate of dispersal from homotypically conditioned medium and fresh flour. In I, castaneum the rate of dispersal increased with selection, as com- pared to dispersal from fresh medium, while it was depressed in I, confusum. Selection, therefore, served only to modulate the normal response of each species. (I, confusum normally aggregates in homo- typically or heterotypically conditioned flour, whereas I, castaneum is normally repelled by both.) Ogden's results clearly indicate a ,genetic component to dispersal behavior. However, it is not clear Inhether the genetic component relates to increased mobility or increased preference for preferred medium. The extent of selection, such as Ogden performed, in natural INqulations and its consequences to population dynamics is largely 18 unknown. However, some work has been done in this area. Wellington (1957), using a behavioral test for the ability of the tent cater- pillar (Malucosoma pluviale) to move towards a light source when iso— lated, found two distinct behavioral populations. Type I larvae were capable of independent, directed movements as isolates and in groups. Type II larvae consisted of larvae that were somewhat active if other larvae were present, larvae that were almost always disoriented, and larvae which hardly moved at all. The consequences of these behaviors to spatial distribution will be discussed later. The relevant point at this time is that these larval types have a genetic component related to mobility differences. Since such differences were also found in the wild, Wellington's results are not a laboratory artifact. Since it was not my intention to investigate the genetic factors in the spatial distribution of Galleria larvae, sex and genetics were controlled as closely as possible. For purposes of my experiments, I attempted to keep genetic variability, relating to spatial distrib- ution, constant. As discussed in Chapter II, new stocks of larvae were periodically introduced into the breeding colony and behavioral tests utilized as indicators of whether response to biological conditioning fluctuates. The results of these tests indicate a constant response over time. These results demonstrate that variability in response differed over time, but the average behavioral response remained con— stant over the period tested. Sexual differences were of no concern in my experiments with (killeria. As near as anyone has been able to determine, Galleria larvae exhibit no sexual dimorphoism. For purposes of the 19 experiments in this dissertation, therefore, Galleria larvae are assumed to be sexually undifferentiated. However, there may be a behavioral dimorphism relating to sexual differences which may account for a component of the observed changing variance over time. This was not investigated. Density Factors in Spatial Distribution One of the variables investigated in this dissertation is the effect conspecifics have on one another in terms of spatial distrib- Iition and if the results of such animal-animal interactions are altered as age, number of organisms, and degree of biological conditioning are unanipulated. The discussion at this point will consider density «iependent factors commonly associated with population spatial patterns. latter we will discuss similar considerations in terms of biological Ccnuiitioning. The density related literature in invertebrates, Particularly insects, is of main concern to my studies. However, relevant hypotheses derived with rodents will be briefly touched upon. Stickle (1946) and Calhoun and Webb (1953), using small mam- I“5‘15, have shown that stability of spatial distribution is related to 't}\e number of animals in an area. Animals in surrounding areas tend 1:5) move to vacated areas following removal of the residents. Intro- CIllction of animals into already populated areas causes them to dis- ID‘ETSG (Calhoun 1948) or to so disrupt the population that a temporary CI N_t Fa. t, N t CCHTfidence Regions on Mean Vector Simultaneous Confidence (Ellipsoid intervals) Intervals for Linear Compounds of Means Fig. 3.1 2 Hotelling's One Sample T Statistic. 88 The calculations necessary to test the null hypothesis are derived from a matrix of N independent observation vectors and t time units. Let A be such a raw data matrix, such that l . . . t r" 7 1 a11 . . . a1t A = . N aN1 . . . aNt and each entry in the matrix is some measurable characteristic of interest over time. It is necessary that N > t since the degrees of freedom for the test statistic are t and N-t, and if N f-t this test cannot be made. 1 From this matrix are calculated the X, B, B', C, S, and S7 matrices, where 2 X! II the mean VBCtOI‘ Whose entries are the means calculated from each column in A. For example, X = [a1, a2, . . . at] / N ~ B = the matrix resulting when each entry in X has been sub- tracted from each entry in the corresponding columns of A. 1 . t 1 311 a1 alt at B = . N aNl-al aNt-at __ .__J 89 B' = the transpose of B C = the sums of squares and cross-products matrix calculated as 13'? S = the variance-covariance matrix calculated as C/N-l 8'1 = the inverse of S For the one sample test it is necessary to be able to determine, a_priori, an expected mean vector (30) against which the calculated mean vector (i) is to be compared. The test statistic is 2 -l - - = -—- ' _ T N(§ 50) (§ §o)° 2CD When the null hypothesis is true, the quantity _ N—t 2 F ' t(N-1) T has the F distribution with degrees of freedom t and N-t. 2 t(N—l) - - < ———.——— - = If T __ N-t Fa, t, N-t’ Ho. 5 50° 2 t(N-l) . - - If T > N-t Fa; t, N-t’ Ho. 5 # Eo' If the null hypothesis is not rejected, it is possible to calculate a confidence region for the mean vector as follows: (N-l)t N-t a; t, N-t N(E-go)'§-l(g-Eo)‘: This confidence region is represented by a single number which T2 “Knlld have to be bigger than before the null hypothesis could be rej ected. This value will change as N and t fluctuate, or as the 0“level is varied. If the null hypothesis is rejected, simultaneous confidence 11l‘tervals for linear compounds of means are calculated as follows: 9O - 1 (N-l)t : _ | ._____ v 1 s25 \Igeée [it Fa, 1., NJ 5.211153, 1 , (N-l)t JET? §§ [N—t F01; t, N—J where, a = a vector the experimenter specifies, and ~ +- Z> Box's Homogeneity of Variance Test M=N LogeISI- Inverse of Covariance Matrix (S-l) éf”””’ Z(Vk LogelSkl) ~ k 1 Calculate: 2 = N1N2 N + N \L 1 2 Calculate: N1 + N2 - t 2 = T (N1 + N2 - 2)t i Hypothesis Testing °‘ 51 = 52 _ _ -1 _ - _ 1 - T (5 52W (E15) F H0 is not rejected if H0 is rejected if T2 < (N1+N2-2)t 2 (N1+N2-2)t ~—_________ > _ N1+N2-t'1 Fa; t, N1+N2‘t-1 T N1+N2-t-1 Fa; t, N1+N2-t-l ElliPSOid Confidence Intervals Simultaneous Confidence Intervals or the Mean Differences for Linear Compounds of Mean Differences Fig. 3.2 2 Test for Parallelism of two Curves. Hotelling's Two Sample T Ho: Cu = Cu ~~ ~~ Statistic. 95 Table 3.1 Components of the Two Sample T2 Statistic Sample 1 Sample 2 Sample Size N1 N2 Mean vector X1=[x11, . . . Xlt] X2=[x21, x2t] Matrix of sums of C C squares 8 products ~l ~2 Pooled covariance 1 matrix § — N1 + N2 2 (91 + 92) The test statistic is N N 2 _ l 2 — — , -l - - T ’ N1 + N2 (51'52) § (51'52) and the quantity N1 + N2 - t - l 2 = (N1 + N2 - 2)t T F has the F distribution with degrees of freedom t and N1 + N2 - t - 1. It is important to note that the two raw data matrices being cOmPaI‘ed must be of equal dimension, that is N1 = N2 and t1 = t2. If the dimensions are unequal it becomes impossible to determine the degrees of freedom for the test statistic, or to obtain the pooled (N + N - 2)t nce matrix. If T E'Nl + N2 _ t _ 1 Fa; t, N1 + N2 _ t _ 1, tfm:nu11 hypothesis is not rejected, and the confidence region for the n1 . . ea“ dlff'erence is 96 _ _ 6) _1 _ _ 0) N1 + N2 ’(N1 + N2 - 2)t (X—x-'S(x-X- _<_———-—-—— F. -1 ~2 ~ ~ ~ ~2 ~ NlN2 N1 + N2 - t - 1 a, t, N1 + N2 -t—l, where 6 = the population mean of (Kl—X2). If > (N1 + N2 - 2)t F N1 + N2 - t - l a; t, N1 + N2 - t - l, rejected and the simultaneous confidence intervals about the vector 2 T the null hypothesis is of mean differences is N +N (N1+N2—2)t - - 2 a'(X -X ) - a'Sa F . < a0 < ~ ~1 ~ ~ ~~ NlNz N1+N2"t-l (I, t, N1+N2"t"1 — ~~ — N +N (N +N -2)t - - l 2 1 2 a'(X -X ) + a'Sa F . ~ ~ ~2 \ ~ ~~ NlN2 N1+N2-t-l a, t, N1+Nz-t-l where (N1+N2-2)t F = T2 N1+N2-t-l a; t, N1+N2-t-1 a; t, N1+N2-t-l. A correlation matrix may be calculated as in the one sample Case, except now it is calculated from the pooled variance-covariance matrix. By pooling the two sums of squares and cross products matrices we have assumed that we have homogeneity of variances and thus need not calculate a correlation matrix for each covariance matrix. This assumption will be dealt with shortly. If the null hypothesis is rejected, a profile analysis may be performed on the two response mean vectors, by asking if the popu- lation mean profiles are similar, in the sense that adjacent curve segments are parallel. In behavioral development, for example, parallel curves would indicate similar behaviors, even if at different -leVels. The null hypothesis is Ho: Cul = Cuz. 97 The test statistic is N N 2 _ l 2 - - -l - - T - m— Q‘.1‘§2)'9'(E§§') 9951252) 1 2 where C = a (K-l) x k patterned matrix, having the effect of sub- 7 tracting each row in the matrix from the preceding row. If T2 5' N1+N2-t Fa: t-l N +N -t the null hypothesis is not (N1+N2-2)(t-1) ’ ’ l 2 rejected. An analysis specifically for repeated measures has been worked out for Hotelling's procedure (Morrison 1969, p. 133) in which serial correlation is handled internally by the test statistic. However, this procedure has not been worked out for the case of two samples, has no profile analysis and is limited in scope as to the questions it will answer. The maximum number of experimental groups that can be analyzed bY‘Pkrtelling's two—sample test is two. This means that multiple comparisons must be performed for more than two samples, and presents theProblem of determining an appropriate a-level for testing the null hYPOthesis. This is up to the experimenter, but must be decided 3 331231, If in 100 comparisons, for example, 20 tests turned out to be Significant at the a = .05 level, then 5 of those tests would be eXPeCted to be significant on the basis of chance alone. The a—level ShOHLd therefore be set at most at the a = 0.01 when such comparisons are attempted. There are several multivariate tests in the literature which allow Many treatment groups to be simultaneously compared. There are, 'however, two problems with such tests. Firstly, they are much less 98 powerful than Hotelling's T2 test even when the a-level is adjusted. Secondly, if the results of such a test indicate significant dif- ferences, the experimenter has no way of knowing which treatment groups caused the difference. In such a case the only recourse would be pair-wise comparisons to pull apart the differences. Example 3.2.--In the experimental design of Example 3.1 sup- pose that we now have two experimental groups each of which is pre- sented with a different degree of biological conditioning and we are interested in asking if larvae respond identically to the different degrees of conditioning. The raw data matrices are t1 t2 t3 t1 t2 t3 1 II. 1 1‘ 1'0 0 0' 1 1 1 0 0 0 . 1 1 1 . 0 0 0 1 0 1 1 1 1 51 = 1 1 1 A2 = . 0 0 0 1 1 0 ~ 0 0 0 . 1 1 1 . 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 10 Li 1 31 10_1_ 0 .13 31 = [0.9 0.9 0.9]. $2 = [0.5 0.4 0.3]. The pooled coVariance matrix is f— a .189 .106 .078 .106 .183 .094 L_._078 .094 ~1§Z. The quantity (XI-12) = [0.4 0.5 0.6]. The test statistic is 2 1 8.1347 -3.8829 -l.6138 0.4 'r =-£Igl§l%%- [0.4 0.5 0.6] -3.8829 9.5404 —3.5564 0.5 * -l.6l38 -3.5564 8.7436 0.6 11.8652, and _ 10 - _ F - T33§13180 _32)31 (11.5156) = 3.5156, d.f. = 3. 16. 99 The tabulated critical value of F0.05; 3, 16 = 3.24. Since our calculated F is larger than the critical value we reject the null hypothesis and conclude that larvae respond differently to different degrees of conditioning. The simultaneous confidence intervals are calculated as they were in Example 3.1, except the two sample formula is used. For example if we wanted to look at the first time period, then a - [l 0 0] and the confidence interval becomes 0.7212. ~ Knowing that the response means are different we now want to test for parallelism between the two curves. This is done by first constricting the (k-l)(k) patterned matrix, C, as follows 1 —1 0 20 ll 0 1 -1 The test statistic is 'l'! _. ( )(1 ) 0 1 0 S O I -| O ' - l 1 0 '1 1 —1 0 0.4 -1 1 0 1 -1 0.5 0 —1 0.6 1.0, and p = 10 + 10 - 3 10 + 10 - 2)(3 — 1) (1'0) ( 0.4722, d.f. = 2, 17. The tabulated critical value for F0.OS; 2, 17 = 3.59. Since the calculated F is less than the tabulated value the null hypothesis $anPOt-13e rejected and we conclude that the curves are parallel. This cipdlcates that although the response mean vectors are significantly 1fferent, these organisms are behaving the same way but at different 1:;:::- That is, there is no interaction between response and age of %¥H§f§fiflflmptions Underlying W5 Procedure The assumptions underlying Hotelling's T2 Statistic are: (1) multivariate nOrmal distribution, (2) equality of 100 variance-covariance matrices, and (3) serially uncorrelated data. These assumptions must be satisfied. Multivariate Normal Distribution Just as in the univariate case, data for multivariate analysis must be normally distributed. Procedures for testing this assumption are handled by Sokal and Rohlf (1969) and appropriate transformations are discussed, which are the best ways to correct for lack of normality. If non-normality is present, then the probability value for F is an underestimate which causes errors in the direction of announcing too many significant results (Cochran 1947). In addition to its effects on the validity of tests of significance, non-normality is accompanied by a loss in efficiency in the estimation of treatment effects and corresponding loss of power in the F- or t-tests. Sokal and Rohlf (1969), however, show that the consequences of non-normality are not too serious. Only very skewed distributions would have a marked effect on the significance level of the F-test or on the efficiency of the design. Given certain prerequisites, data that is not normally dis- tributed may be used with Hotelling's T2 Statistic. For example, the data discussed in Examples 3.1 and 3.2 is binomial data, which is a discrete distribution not a continuous one. It would seem, therefore, that this data should be transformed to fit a normal distribution. fhawever, the crucial part of Hotelling's test is the difference Vector (3'80) in the one sample or (81-32) in the two sample. Although the raw data is discrete, i.e., ones and zeros, the data being tested comes from (X-Xo) or (XI-1(2) which may yield any number 101 between 1 and 0. This then becomes a continuous distribution, as N approaches m. The Central Limit Theorem states that as sample size increases, the means of samples drawn from a population of any distribution will approach the normal distribution. There are not very many statis- ticians, however, willing to categorically state how big the sample size must be before this theorem is true, but a general rule of thumb is > 30. However, for illustrative purposes smaller samples are described in Examples 3.1 and 3.2. For actual test data calculate, a_priori, a sample size necessary to satisfy the assumption of normality based upon the Central Limit Theorem. The procedure (personal communi- cation with Dr. John Gill of Michigan State University) is N = 3/W(1-fi) where N sample size a any value the experimenter would like to be able to differentiate. This might be done by picking the smallest difference one wishes to be able to detect. Let us suppose we have run a pilot experiment in the two choice situations described earlier to determine Galleria's preference to the highest degree of conditioning we intend to test, and find that the largest response mean in the mean vector is 0.96. This is W. It is now possible to calculate the N that would be necessary, given that W = 0.96, to satisfy the assumption of normality. The result is N 3/0.96(1-0.96) 78.125. Therefore, an N greater than 78 would be required. 102 Equality of Variance— Covariance Matrices A test for heterogeneous variance is not necessary in the one sample test. Hotelling (1931) originally devised the statistic with- out the underlying assumption that the variances within a group are homogeneous. This is reasonable since the test is designed to examine multivariate factors among which homogeneous variances would not be expected. When comparing two experimental groups, however, the assumption of equal variance-covariance matrices must be satisfied. Just as in the univariant case where the assumption is made that the observations are normally distributed about their population mean values with con- stant variance, so an analogous assumption that the variates are multinormally distributed about their mean values with constant variance-covariance matrices is made in the multivariate analysis of variance. Cochran (1947) has shown that if ordinary analysis of variance methods are used when the true error variance differs from one observation to another there will be a loss of efficiency in the estimates of treatment effects as well as a loss of sensitivity in tests of significance. The validity of the F-test for all treatments is probably least affected, but t—tests from a pooled error may seri- ously distort the significance levels. Box (1950) has devised a method for testing the assumption of homogeneous variances in multivariate analysis. Example 3.3.--Let us return to Example 3.2 and calculate Box's test for homogeneous variances. M = N 16ge |§l — E (Vk loge |§k|). 103 where N = Vk’ the total of the degrees of freedom, Sk = the unbiased estimate of each covariance matrix being ~ compared, Vk = degrees of freedom, S = the pooled variance or covariance matrix, and II = the symbol for determinants. In Example 3.2, loge|§1l = —6.948348, v1 = 9 logeISZI = -6.1862086, v2 = 9 ioge|§ | = —5.9251272, N = 18 M = 18(-6.l862086)—9(-6.948348)-9(-6.1862086) M = 6.8592. Calculate 2 _ 2t — 3t — 1 (z 1/v1 — 1/N), l - 6(t+l)(k-l) k where k = the number of matrices being compared. A Fl = 1/2 (k-l)t(t+l). Therefore, A _ 2(3)2 + 3(3)-l 1 ‘ 6(3+1)(l-1) 0.1806, and '11 ll 1/2 (1-1)3(3+1) = 6, and (l-A1)M is distributed as X2 with f degrees of freedom, or 1 (1-0.1806)6.8592 = 5.6204 In this case A1 = 0.1806, f1 = 6, (1-A1)M = 5.6204 is referred 2 . . . tc> tables of X with 6 degrees of freedom. The probability for the Occurrence of a value as great or greater than this, when the variances and covariances are in fact homogeneous from one group to 104 the next, is thus between 0.5 and 0.1, and there is therefore no reason to doubt the homogeneity of the data. The chi-squared approximation is good if k and/or t do not exceed 4 or 5 or if some of the degrees of freedom are small. If, however, k and/or t are greater than 5 or the degrees of freedom are large, Box proposes an F—distribution approximation where fit-muz) 21 v 2 2 A2 ”L6(k-1) k / k] ‘ UN and M/b is referred to an F-table with f and f degrees of freedom where 1 2 f + 2 f f2 = A1 A 2 and b = 1 - A -If /f 2'1 1 12 It will be noticed that Box's approximation allows the simul— taneous analysis of any number of matrices, but does not provide a method for distinguishing which matrix is causing problems when the test is heterogeneous. Therefore, it is necessary to perform pair- wise comparisons in this event. If the test proves to be heterogeneous there is no approximate method available for Hotelling's two-sample test and the test should be run as if the variances were homogeneous. If this is done a decision must be made pertaining to the validity of the significance test. If two covariance matrices are heterogeneous the pooled covari- ance matrix will be smaller than one and larger than the other. There will thus be a loss in sensitivity because the variances have been inflated, making it more difficult to reject the null hypothesis. Therefore, we can be confident of a significance test in which the ruill hypothesis has been rejected, at the usual a-level of 0.05. If a_priori an experimenter suspects finding heterogeneity of vrrriance he should increase the sample size. Ito and Schull (1964) Turve demonstrated that, for Hotelling's two-sample statistic, as N 105 approaches infinity the test statistic and significance tests are unaffected by heterogeneous variances. The general rule of thumb is that N must be greater than or equal to 30 for this to hold true. Serially Uncorrelated Data The greatest difficulty with longitudinal data is determining if the error terms are correlated and what to do if they are. However, it does not necessarily follow that longitudinal data is correlated. If the assumption of independence is satisfied, the analytical pro- cedure is valid whether or not the observations themselves are serially correlated. Cochran (1947) has shown that if correlation exists the following are the consequences: (1) if the correlation is positive, the treatment means are less accurate than the mean of an independent series, but are estimated to be more accurate, and (2) if correlation is negative, these conditions are reversed. Durbin and Watson (1950, 1951) and Watson (1955) have worked out several regression models for testing the assumption of serially uncorrelated data. There are several of these tests, depending on the kind of data or experimental design, and the reader is directed to the original papers. However, for the purposes of my design and resultant discrete data the Durbin-Watson "One-and-Two—Way Classifi- cation was used (see pp. 166-168 of Durbin—Watson 1951). The cal— CUIations are much too laborious to be presented at this time, but entail a regression model consisting of u + oi + Bj + 2.. Y.. ij 1] Where . . . . h the observation in the ith column and Jt row, .< ll 11' 106 + B .. co 5 ants and 1110113 n t ’ zij the error term. Least squares estimates of u, ai and 8i are calculated and designated as m, Ai-m and bj-m where m = the sample mean of all observations, . . .th Ai = the mean observation in the 1 column, . . .th bj = the mean observation in the 3 row. Thus the residuals are given by Z.. = Y.. - A. - b. + m, and 1] 1J 1 J the test is made by calculating £(AZ..)2 c1=——————1J 2 , XZ .. 13 where AZ.. = the first differences of the residuals when arranged as a single time series. The calculated d-value is compared to a lower (dL) and upper (dU) range of critical values. If d < dL there is positive correlation° , if d > no correlation exists. If d < d < d the test is incon— L-— ._ U elusive. Durbin and Watson (1951) have calculated several tables of upper and lower d-values. However, most of my research deals with large matrices and I had to generate other tables in situations where k' (the number of time units) was greater than 5. This was done on the Computer using Durbin and Watson's (1950) formulas on p. 427. Regardless of the outcome of the Durbin-Watson test for serial (xxrrelation, Hotelling's T2 statistic is run normally. If correlation exists, then by examining the variance-covariance matrices we can make a (decision about the significance tests' validity. Positive serial 107 correlation means that the variances are over—estimates making it difficult to reject the null hypothesis, whereas negative correlation means the variances have been under-estimated making it easy to reject the null hypothesis. Therefore, with positive correlation the significance tests should be run at the a = 0.05 level and at the a = 0.01 level when negative correlation is the problem. However, tests for negative correlation are rarely run unless the experimenter has an a_priori reason to suspect it. For example, it is a common practice in econometric work (Durbin and Watson 1951) to analyze the first differences of the observations rather than the observations themselves, on the ground that the serial correlation of the transformed errors is likely to be less than that of the original errors. It is possible that the transformation has over-corrected, thus introducing negative serial correlation into the transformed errors . Summary Statistical procedures, and the assumptions underlying them, are presented for the multivariate analysis of longitudinal data. Hotelling's One and Two Sample T2 Statistics for Multivariate Analysis are outlined, illustrating a procedure for treating time as a multi- dimensional variate. Emphasized are procedures for testing the assumptions of homogeneous variances and non-serially correlated data. The major objective is to arm behavioral biologists with the Imecessary analytical techniques enabling logical decisions about tests of significance on repeated individual measurements over time. CHAPTER IV BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND SPATIAL DISTRIBUTION IN GALLERIA MELLONELLA (L.) LARVAE Introduction Stimuli affecting spatial distribution are such environmental parameters as light, temperature and humidity (Allee 1926; Heatwole 1962; Surtees 1963a, 1964e 6 f; Friedlander 1964; Warburg 1964), food (Park 1938; Brown 1946; Loschiavo 1952; Wynne-Edwards 1962; Beck 1960), sex and genetic factors (Wellington 1957; Naylor 1959; McDonald and Fitting 1965; Ogden 1969), and density (Long 1953; Wellington 1957; Naylor 1959; Surtees 1963a, b 8 c, 1964a, b, c, e 6 f). This paper, however, is concerned with the relationship between biological conditioning and spatial distribution. Biological conditioning refers to changes produced in a medium by organisms living therein. The results of such homotypic conditioning are physiological or behavioral. The physiological effects of biological conditioning have been extensively studied (Uvarov 1938; Allee 1931, 1934; Adolph 1931; Park 1932, 1934, 1935, 1936a 8 b; Brown 1946, 1951; Long 1953). It has been demonstrated ‘thaI.biological conditioning affords protection from toxic substances, it; involved in control of sex, affects morphological changes and growth, and affects population dynamics, such as natality and 108 109 mortality. Historically, biological conditioning has been considered to be a population phenomenon. It is an expression of population den- sity and is a normal and inevitable result of population growth. As populations grow and maintain themselves, they of necessity modify their environment and the environment in turn modifies them. However, individuals also condition their environment and it has not yet been demonstrated if the effects of population conditioning are different from the effects of individual conditioning. Since biological con- ditioning is a result of the organisms themselves and since the degree of conditioning is a function of the number of organisms present or of the length of time an organism is present, biological conditioning and animal-animal interactions have been considered as an inescapable unit. However, they can be separated by experimental procedures. The effects of either, or the interaction between the two, may alter the behavior of individuals with resulting effects on populations. Biological conditioning has been demonstrated to affect spatial distribution of populations of organisms, particularly invertebrates (see Chapter I). It has been shown in Tribolium (Park and Woolcott 1937; Park 1948) that homotypic conditioning alters the movements of these beetles and that adult populations of Tribolium seem to prefer weakly-conditioned medium. Naylor (1959 and 1965) extended these findings to include animal-animal encounters, such as interactions Iaetween the sexes, and homotypic conditioning, and he found that the distribution of Tribolium confusum and I, castaneum was only explain- able when all three factors were accounted for. The interaction betrween these three variables seems a common phenomenon among 110 invertebrates, particularly insects. For example, Long (1953, 1955) with butterfly larvae and Wellington (1957) with larvae of the tent caterpillar both demonstrated similar interactions. Although biological conditioning affects spatial distribution and distribution may also be altered by other variables, the inter- actions between animal-animal interactions and homotypic conditioning remain confounded. Studies dealing with biological conditioning deal with whole populations, and little, if any, data have been collected on the effects on individual behavior. It is the purpose of this paper to look at the role of homo- typic conditioning in the dispersionary behavior of isolated Galleria mellonella (L.) larvae, the assumption being that before we can under- stand population behavior we must first understand individual behavior. The general hypothesis being tested is that response of isolate larvae to biological conditioning will change as a function of the degree of conditioning and age of the test larva. In testing this hypothesis a bioassay of behavioral preferences to conditioning was developed. Materials and Methods Husbandry Rearing Galleria mellonella is a relatively simple task and was described in Chapter II. All experimental larvae were drawn from tflie colony dishes which were maintained in darkness in incubators at 31:}°C and 75% R.H. Due to the manner in which egg hatches were ‘ulllected (see Chapter II), the age of larvae within any colony dish had a maximum possible variability of 24 hours. It was therefore 111 necessary to have a criteria for consistently determining age from hatch to hatch. Weight of larvae was used as the criteria. Table 4.1 shows the mean weights of 500 larvae, at each age, randomly selected from appropriate colony dishes as well as the corresponding weights of larvae actually used for experimental purposes. Figure 4.1 shows the size distribution of larvae corresponding to Table 4.1. Apparatus The experimental apparatus consisted of 100 x 25 mm plastic petri dishes with two 4:0.05 gram food lumps in each dish. Food was prepared as described in Chapter 11. Within any one dish the food lumps were placed on opposite sides of the dish so that test larvae could be introduced between them and allowed to make a choice. Experimental Procedures The experimental design is shown in Figure 4.2. Four con- ditioning periods (1, 3, 5, and 7 days) and three ages of conditioning larvae (6, 9, and 12 days of age) were used. Preference tests were run with 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae for each conditioning age and/or period. Each box in the design has an identification code (1E, 2B, 3B, and 37C) where E stands for experimental group and C for control group. There is one control for every three experimental groups based on days of conditioning. For example, 37C is the control .for groups 1E, 2E, and SE at one day of conditioning. Each group has a sample size of 80. The date in each box of Figure 4.2 indicates the day on which the test was begun for each group. Due to the size of the 112 Table 4.1 Larval weights corresponding to the larvae in Figure 4.1. Weights were calculated for 500 randomly selected larvae of a particular age as well as for the larvae of a particular age actually used for experimentation. A e Randomly Selected Experimental Larvae (D: 5) [Mean weights(mg) [Mean weights(mg) y .1 standard deviation] :_standard deviation] 6 0.53 :_0.012 0.71 :_0.009 7 1.27 :_0.029 1.49 i 0.010 9 5.29 :_0.090 6.33 :_0.069 11 12.45 :_0.221 12.72 I 0.014 12 22.14 :_0.710 22.56 :_0.320 13 34.40 1 0.760 33.82 :_0.221 15 57.60 i 0.700 55.40 :_0.250 Figure 4.1 Frc>m right to left, these are 6, 7, 9, ll, 12, 13, G 15 day old larvae “Seed for conditioning and testing preference for conditioning. Note the: size distribution of the larvae. 113 .::h mez much» now Houucou new macaw Amy Hmucosfiuomxo some con: came onu ecu agenda :owumowmfiucoew on» «cwzocm .cuwmov Huucoafluomxm ~.e engage ee\N\e ee\m~\m ee\e\e ee\e\e one one uee ume - 2 v ee\~\e , ee\~\o ee\m\e ee\m~\m ee\m~\m ee\m~\m ee\e\e ee\e\e ee\e\e ee\m\e ee\e\e ee\e\e Ana men mmm men men mmn mHm men mew mew mew mew mmm qua - . o , ee\H~\m ee\-\e ee\n\m ee\e~\o I. uee one owe uHe - - - = H eh\HN\m ee\H~\m en\H~\m ee\mm\e ee\-\e ee\N~\e ee\n\m ee\n\m ee\n\m eh\e~\e ee\e~\e ee\e~\e In. mem mnm mNN mam mom me” me" meH meH mmH meH meg an. V eh\efi\m ee\e\e ee\na\e ee\e~\e “nu 8e eon 68 oh A k V ee\ea\m ee\wa\m ee\ea\m ee\w\e ee\e\e ee\e\e ee\n~\e ee\mfi\e ee\nH\e ee\efi\e ee\efi\e ee\e~\e an. mNH mHH meH me we we we mm me mm mu me a. e e a. o e S e e a. e e A. emu k m m — defer 02_ZO_.—_OZOU *0 mummvmo 114 experimental design and the total number of larvae required (total N = 3840) it was necessary to test groups at different times and with different populations (hatches) of larvae. Therefore, the test date for every block of three experimental groups and their control was randomly determined, a_priori. Every four groups form a natural entity of days of conditioning, age of conditioning larvae, and age of test larvae and were therefore run at the same time. For example, 1E, 2E, 3E, and 37C or 19E, 20E, 21E, and 43C were each run as a unit. Experimental group 8E has been chosen to elucidate the experi- mental procedures. Food was prepared as discussed in Chapter II, Table 2.1, and 160, 4:0.05 gram food lumps were weighed on a Mettler P120 Scale. These food lumps were flattened on wax paper until they were approximately 6 mm thick and 157 mm in circumference. One food lump was then placed in each of 160, 150 x 25 mm plastic petri dishes, the lump touching the periphery of the dish. In 80 of these dishes a conditioning larva was added, in this case a 9-day old larva. The other dishes have no conditioning larva. All dishes were then put in a Jamesway Single Stage Incubator-Hatcher, Model 2528, for the length of time conditioning was to occur; in this case 5 days. At the end of the 5 day period the following manipulations were performed. The conditioning larva, which is now 14 days old, is :removed from each of the 80 dishes by gently tapping the silk tunnels ill has spun with a pair of forceps. This causes the larva to poke iits head out and it can be carefully removed with forceps. Once all 1arVéle are removed, the 80 non-conditioned food lumps are placed one in eaiczh of the conditioned dishes opposite the conditioned lump. This 115 is done wearing surgical gloves since pilot experiments indicated larvae will selectively respond to food handled with bare hands. The distance between food lumps is approximately 35 mm across the center of the dish. A small black dot is placed on the side of the petri dish opposite one of the food lumps for identification purposes. The food lumps receiving the identification dot were randomly selected so as to alleviate the possibility of response to the dots. An identifi- cation number was also put on the top of each dish. Once the two food lumps are in the same dish, a single, in this case 7 day old, larva of proper size is removed from the appro- priate colony dish and placed in the petri dish between the lumps and the top is placed on each dish. The total procedure of removing the conditioning larvae, adding the plain food lumps, and seeding all 80 dishes with a test larva takes 5 minutes. At the end of the 5 minute period the location of each larva is recorded and the dishes placed in a Napco (Model 330) incubator at 32:1°C and 75% R.H. in darkness. The incubator holds 8 shelves of 80 dishes per shelf with the dishes stacked 5 deep. Every 12 hours the dishes were removed from the incubator and data collected as to which food lump the larvae were residing in. The dependent variable, therefore, is position of the larvae over time. Except during the wandering phase, larvae were never found between .fbcni lumps. Every 24 hours the position of the dishes on each shelf as Well as shelf position in the incubator was randomized to prevent anyllarval responses to discontinuities of temperature, humidity, or light: within the incubator. 116 In order to record position of the larvae it was necessary to bring the dishes into the light for 5 minutes, every 12 hours. There is some indication that Galleria larvae are photonegative and I may have been recording the preferred food lump in which to escape from light. However, differences in response to degree of conditioning are still evident. Data were collected every 12 hours until pupation occurred, at which time the experiment was terminated. The control group for SE, namely 39C, was set up exactly as the experimental group except that none of the food lumps were con- ditioned. The controls, therefore, control for food differences and the handling of one of the food lumps at the end of the conditioning period. The response of a larva in a control group will be random if these variables have no effect on distribution or preferences. Every group in Figure 4.2 was set up exactly as just described, the only differences being the number of days of conditioning, age of conditioning larvae, and age of test larvae. For purposes of ease in referring to this experimental design and manipulations, I will refer either to the group number, 1E or 10E, or I will use a CAT-value, where C = the number of conditioning days, A.= age of conditioning larva, and T = age of test larva. Therefore, CAT = l, 6, 7 refers to an experiment in which 1 day of conditioning was done by a 6 day old larva and the response of a 7 day old larva was; tested. For group 85, CAT = 5, 9, 7; for group 27E, CAT = 1, 12, 13 and so on. If multiple comparisons are desired, the CAT-value can [)3 used to indicate these as well. For example, if we wanted to 100k .at l, 3, 5, and 7 days of conditioning done by a 6 day old larva 117 at the start of conditioning and tested with a 7 day old larva then CAT = (1,3, 5, 7), 6, 7. In all Tables and Figures in this paper test larvae are referred to as 7, 11, or 13 days old. It must be remembered, however, that these are the ages when a particular experiment is started, and that, for example, four days into an experiment a 7 day old larva is 11 days old, an 11 day old larva is 15 days old, and a 13 day old larva is 17 days old. The abscissars in all graphs in this paper are calibrated in days, following initiation of the test not age of test larva. Analysis The dependent variable chosen for analysis within each exper- imental group is the mean response to conditioned food over time. Each experimental group has 80 dishes with 1 larva per dish. If a larva is in the conditioned lump it received a score of 1, if in the non— conditioned (plain) lump it received a 0. In the case of 8B, for example, this would generate an 80 x 20 matrix whose entries are either one's or zero's, 80 being the number of dishes or larvae and 20 being the number of 12 hour intervals. A mean for each 12 hour interval can be calculated which generates a l x 20 vector whose entries are the mean number of larvae in conditioned food at each 12 hour time interval. For actual analysis, however, 24 hour intervals ‘werre utilized, since there was no difference in response from AM (morning) to PM (afternoon) readings (Table 4.2). The wandering phase was also not included in the analysis because wandering larvae exhitxited atypical behavior. This was done because some data 118 . So. w H: meoceewe> msoecomouepoxz .wouoo> moanewe>oo pmewwda on» see: much» 0:» Beam ease umou m.vefimamm oco one you nouoo> wouooexo one Q .HeeweeHeeeeeH u He .me. u e we ew.wu :e eee Hn.H u He Hewe. u e we eH.w «HeeH eHewe. ewe. a e ee ew.w a hen eHeHe. enH. ene. eeH. eee. mee. ewH. eHH. eHH. eeH. wew. eeHeeme eeH. ene. HHH. Hee. Hee. HHH. eeH. HeH. weH. wew. ”me : eeH. ene. HeH. Hee. Hee. ewH. Hee. eeH. HHH. wew. we .me.H nen. ewe. ene. eee. wHe. ene. nee. nee. eee. ewn. 2am meH tee.H eH H en H wHe. ewe. nee. wHe. eee. eme. eee. nee. ene. ewn. 24x, eeH. nnH. nnH. meH. eeH. wnH. wnH. enH. eeH. eew. eeHeewe .ne.H an. enn. enn. wen. nen. wHe. een. nen. een. ewe. 2mm en tee.H ee e em». mnn. mnn. wen. hen. emn..wemw we». em»..hme. 2mm. 0 O Hew. eee. Hee. eee. HHH» eee. emH. enH. wew. eww. e H ewe ree.H een. ene. eee. wHe. ene. nee. wHe. nen. ewn. wee. 2e“ we «ee.H ew e een. wHe. ewe. eee. ene. eee. wHe. wen. ewn. wee. z sees 2m emu 2< nwogu ecu eeuooHem xfiaoecmw one: N.e enamem cw museum kucoawnomxo o>~039 NJ 0.3.3. 119 . 30. W 3 ouceeaenH esoecowonepozz .He>HeeHeeoeeHuHe ee. u e we eH.w u ee eee ee.H u He Hewe. u e ee He.w u HeeH eewe * eee. ene. Hee. ene. eee. eeH. eeHeeme Hee.H eee. ewe. wHe. ewe. wHe. wen. sew mew Hne.w wH e wHe. NHe. wHe. ewe. eee. een. 24% eee. Hee. HeH. eeH. eeH. eew. eeHeeee w Hee.H eee. nee. nee. eee. wHe. nee. 2em mew HHe.H eH e wHe. wHe. nee. ewe. eee. nee. 2 ooeeewm>oo ueomwefl on» see: macaw one Scum came peep n.vemeaew eno exp new Hepuo> empooQXe one .ee. u e um eH.w u ee eee ee.H u He ”ewe. u e ee ee.w «HeeH eewm. .o>wm: ocooew mm3 o: «>1 . . H . H eH .ewe. u e ee ee.w u Hen eeHe. eeH. eeH. eeH. eew. eeHeeee w ee.w nen. een. wen. eee. sew mee ew.w eH e nen. nen. enn. eee. 2 ~o ton vow .om .ooH p005 'pUOD UT 08Al81 % exec cw oEHH . e e m e m H We 5 r 1 Ho w.e.wu._.11 801 80* I 'U '9 I O O ,2 12 .701 .70“ “U 'U 5 E 0601 U6 .5 51 o m 0"504 25 i a ,3 H1 «1 “’ 401 4 .—-—.. 195: CAT=5, 6,11 .-———-. 225: CAT=7, 6,11 50 .......... 205: CAT=5, 9,11 3 o----- 255: CAT=7, 9,11 __ 11—-—-11 215: CAT=5,12,11 -ao-—-x 245: CAT=7,12,11 0T v v v u f v V I I v E v r I v v I v T v— I 12345678910 12345678910 Time in Days Figure 4.8 Time in Days 154 Table 4.9 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons of the re- sponse of 13 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held constant at 1, 3, 5, or 7 days. Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 25E vs. 26E 4.931! 0.91 H P 25E vs. 27E 6.79"' 2.86 H P 26E vs. 27E 4.34" 1.78 H P 28E vs. 29E 3.35** H P 28E vs. 30E 3.46* H P 29E vs. 30E 1.33 0.31 H P 31E vs. 325 7.61"' 2.14 H P 31E vs. 33E 6.97 1.68 H P 32E vs. 33E 3.81" 1.12 H P 35E vs. 34E 0.63 0.32 H P 36E vs. 34E 8.52*** H P 36E vs. 358 6.75*** H P F1(4,76)= 2.88 at P = .025. F2(4,155)= 2.88 at P = .025. 5 = = * = = F1(4,76) 3.59 at P .01. F2(4,155) 3.47 at P .01. H ._.._ = *1": , ._. F1(4,76) 5.18 at P .001. F2(4,155) 4.94 at P .001. EV.= expected vector and CV. Test('). covariance matrix for the One Sample “Heterogeneous variances (P 5.05) . l)Mean response vectors are parallel (P 5.05) . 155 Figure 4.9 Mean response vectors of 13 day old test larvae to conditioning done by 6, 9, and 12 day old larvae as days of conditioning are held con- stant at l, 3, 5, or 7 days. A. CAT=1,(6,9,12),13 B. CAT=3,(6,9,12),13 C. CAT=5,(6,9,12),13 D. CAT=7,(6,9,12),13 100‘ A 90‘ 80" H "g / 1E )1 .701 / 'U *‘su‘ C‘. O U 1 1: 60 -1-1 ___.__./ 8 1 g 50 t6 1.] 89 I 40 --—-. 255: CAT=1, 6,15 .----. 265: CAT=1, 9,15 501 5...... 275: CAT=1,12,13 01v.—~‘ ' v v v V "V V 1 2 5 4 5 6 7 8 Time in Days 10m 00 no ‘1 Q \1 9 % Larvae in Cond. Food U1 0‘ Q Q 4:. Q 64 Q L 0T ...—_. 515: CAT=5, 6,15 .---.. 525: CAT=5, 9,15 1...... 555: CAT=5,12,13 ' V 12345678910 Time in Days 156 1001 901 801 70 601 SC % Larvae in Cond. Food 40 9G 00 q \1 Q % Larvae inmCond. Food EL —— 285: CAT=3, 6,15 .--—- 295: CAT=3, 9,15 11....._..1 505: CAT=3,12,13 v v v 12545678910 Time in Days 1001 of...“ I l / Y I—-g\‘) Figure 4.9 50 4 .———-.545: CAT=7, 6,15 H..-»- 555: CAT=7, 9,15 3 x 11565: CAT=7,12,13 545678910 Time in Days 1 2 157 1. Within any particular day of conditioning and age of test larva, response to conditioning increases as the age of the conditioning larva increases (Figure 4.7A, B, C, or D). 2. As days of conditioning increase (Figure 4.7A, B, C, and D) the response to conditioning by a particular age of test larva increases. That is, the response of a 7 day old larva is greater to 7 days of conditioning by a 9 day old larva than it is to 1 day of conditioning by a 9 day old larva. 3. As days of conditioning increase (Figure 4.7A, B, C, and D) there is less discrimination in response to conditioning by the 3 ages of test larvae. For example, the response vectors in Figure 4.7D are at a higher level and more bunched than in Figure 4.7A. Even though these trends are evident in Figures 4.7, 4.8, and 4.9, the F-values in Tables 4.7, 4.8, and 4.9 indicate that there are only a few significant differences in response to conditioning done by the three ages of conditioning larvae within a constant test age and constant days of conditioning. This may be a function of the ages of conditioning larvae chosen for these experiments, i.e., 6, 9, and 12 days old. If the ages had been more spread out, i.e., 5, 10, and 15 days old, the significant differences should have been more numerous. However, this lack of significant differences in response to conditioning by different aged larva is more probably a function of heterogeneous variances. The Fl-values in Tables 4.7, 4.8, and 4.9 exhibit many more significant differences among comparisons. 158 The problem with the response vectors for groups 12E, 24B, and 36B is again evident. III. (getween-Group Comparisons of the Effects of Larval Age on Response to Conditioning W Table 4.3 indicates that there may be age differences relating to larval response to homotypic conditioning. If such differences exist, the F-values for the response to the same degree of conditioning by the same aged conditioning larva should vary, i.e., increase, as age of test larvae increases. For example, in Table 4.3 the F-values for response of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to 3 days of con- ditioning done by a 9 day old conditioning larva (groups 5E, 17E, and 29E) are 8.59, 17.70, and 25.35, respectively. This indicates that older larvae respond at a higher level than younger larvae to the same degree of conditioning. Since the response vectors for 7, 11, and 13 day old larvae are not compatible time-wise, only the first 4 days of response were used for each comparison. The results of these comparisons are shown in Tables 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12 and the corresponding Figures 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12. All comparisons exhibit heterogeneous variances and all comparisons are parallel. With the Two—Sample comparison (F2-values) only significant differences in response due to age show up when there are 7 days of conditioning done by 6, 9, or 12 day old larvae. Groups 128, 24B, and 365 have been ignored because they are atypical, as previously discussed. Therefore, age differences prevail when CAT=7,6(7,13) [comparison 10E versus 345, Table 4.10, Figure 4.10D], CAT=7,6(11,13) 159 Table 4.10 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons for age differences in response to conditioning. The responses of test lar- vae (7, 11, and 13 days old) to 1, 3, 5, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 6 day old larva are tested. Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 135 vs. 15 8.33"' 1.26 H P 18 vs. 25E 12.19"' 1.82 H P 13E vs. 25E 1.55 0.72 H P 4E vs. 16E 3.00' 1.36 H P 4E vs. 28E 0.51 0.13 H P 28E vs. 16E 2.53 1.24 H P 7E vs. 195 3.29' 1.35 H P 7E vs. 31E 7.97"' 2.77 H P 195 vs. 31E S.85"' 2.32 H P 105 vs. 225 2.16 0.89 H P 108 vs. 34E 4.95** H P 22E vs. 345 6.35** H P Fl(4 76): 2.96 at P = .025. F2(4 155): 2.88 at P = .025. 0 = = * = . = F1(4,76) 3.59 at P .01. F2(4’155) 3.47 at P .01. " = = *‘k‘k = : F1(4,76) 5.18 at P .001. F2(4,155) 4.94 at P .001. EV.= expected vector and CV.= covariance matrix for the One Sample Test('). HHeterogeneous variances (p E .05) . pMean response vectors are parallel (P £305). 160 Figure 4.10 Mean response vectors of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to l, 3, S, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 6 day old larva. A. CAT=1,6,(7,11,13) 5. CAT=3,6,(7,11,13) O CAT=5,6,(7,11,13) C3 CAT=7,6,(7,11,13) 161 100' A 1001 B 90‘ 90‘ 80‘ 80' 'U “:3 8 LL. “\ u: 701 . 701 ‘~..._ 5 '3 x ,0 8 J 60‘ .‘E. .5 C) a) 3 350‘ H 5 3 .1 «99 °‘° 4d .———-. 15: CAT=1,6, 7 .._._. 45: CAT=3,6, 7 0, -.......155: CAT=1,6,11 30 o----ol6E: CAT=3,6,11 3.“ 11 112515: CAT=1,6,13 fl 111—12813: CAT=3,6,13 J,,--.--...J...-----._. 12345678910 12345678910 Time in Days Time in Days lomc 100‘ go 90 1 801 1380 "U E: E .701 67d I 'O 8 5 J U6d U6 .5. .5 Q) Q) ‘3 Cd :50- - :35 :3 :3 o\° do 4d 4 r———. 75: CAT=5,6, 7 O--—-—o 10E: CAT=7,6, 7 O-c---o 195: CAT=5,6,11 3 .......... 225: CAT=7,6,11 30 5—1 515: CAT=S,6,13 x 8545: CAT=7,6,13 db OLV V V U 1" V V U I V 6f V I V V V T V I V 1 12345678910 1234567891 Time in Days Time in Days Figure 4.10 162 Table 4.11 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons for age differences in response to conditioning. The responses of test lar- vae (7, 11, and 13 days old) to l, 3, S, or 7 days of conditioning by a 9 day old larva are tested. Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 2E vs. 14E 4.06" 1.72 H P 25 V5. 265 6.52"' 1.90 H P 265 V5. 14 4.71" 1.19 H P 55 V5. 175 5.82"' 2.71 H P 55 V5. 295 5.24"' 1.96 H P 175 V5. 195 5.96"' 1.39 H P 85 V5. 205 1.70 0.62 H P 85 V5. 325 6.15"' 1.96 H P 205 VS. 325 2.64 1.57 H P 115 V3. 235 6.52"' 2.05 H P 115 V5. 355 5.47*** H P 235 V5. 355 9.59'1' 1.30 F1(4,76)= 2.96 at P = .025. 52(4’155)= 2.88 at P = .025. ' F1(4,76)= 3.59 at P = .01. ** 52(4’155)= 3.47 at P - .01. "F1(4,76)= 5.18 at P = .001. ***52(4’155)= 4.94 at P = .001. EV.= expected vector and CV.= covariance matrix for the One Sample Test('). HHeterogeneous variances (P $.05). PMean response vectors are parallel (P 3.05) . 163 Figure 4.11 Mean response vectors of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to l, 3, 5, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 9 day old larva. A. CAT=1,9,(7,11,13) B. CAT=3,9,(7,11,13) C. CAT=5,9,(7,11,13) D. CAT=7,9,(7,11,13) 164 100) A 100‘ B 90‘ 90' ,2. >9 80' 80' / V v p O O o 0 7 u. y LL _ 70 , 70 v v c c o 0 U ‘ U i c 60 c 60 ".4 -H 3 . 8 a 50 i 50' m m 5—1 1—1 o\° o\° 40' 40‘ —. 2E: CAT=1,9, 7 .-———-o 5E: CAT=3,9, 7 3 ....._..14E: CAT=1,9,11 30. ~——-.—.17E: CAT=3,9,11 4L; ‘26E: CAT=1,9,13 __x x29E: CAT=3,9,13 nT-..-.....-J..-..,-- 1234567891 12345678910 Time in Days Time in Days 10% C 1001 It 90' 4/ \§ " 80‘ 'U '0 o O o O U- u. _70‘ . 5 5 8 o u ‘ u c 60 c -r-4 "'1 550' {.00 c!) (11 .J -J °°44r "40 ——-———~ 8E: CAT=5,9, 7 ._————. 11E: CAT=7,9, 7 -----20E: CAT=5,9,11 30‘ ~------- 23E: CAT=7,9,11 3%: 4525: CAT=5,9,13 "__ v: x 555: CAT=7,9,13 CL I I I I ' V I I V ()T—' I I I U I I I I ' 12345678910 12345678910 Time 1n Days Figure 4.11 111116 1n Days 165 Table 4.12 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons for age differences in reSponse to conditioning. The responses of test lar- vae (7, 11, and 13 days old) to 1, 3, 5, and 7 days of conditioning by a 12 day old larva are tested. ‘ Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 3E vs. 15E 0.63 0.33 H P 3E vs. 27E 2.44 1.20 H P lSE vs. 27E 1.73 0.73 H P 6E vs. 18E 9.46"' 2.20 H P 6E vs. 30E 4.04" 1.36 H P 30E vs. 18E 6.47"' 2.34 H P 9E vs. 21E 3.69" 0.97 H P 9E vs. 33E 7.33"' 2.77 H P 21E vs. 335 5.76"' 1.81 H P 12E vs. 24E 5.30*** H P 12E vs. 36E 2.43 0.72 H P 36E vs. 24E 3.95** H P F1(4,76)= 2.96 at P = .025. F2(4,155)= 2.88 at P = .025. ' = = * = = F1(4,76) 3.59 at P .01. F2(4,155) 3.47 at P .01. '1 -_- = *** = : F1(4,76) 5.18 at P .001. F2(4,155) 4.94 at P .001. EV.= expected vector and CV.= covariance matrix for the One Sample Test('). HHeterogeneous variances (PS.05). PMean response vectors are parallel (P:E.05). 166 Figure 4.12 Mean response vectors of 7, 11, and 13 day old test larvae to l, 3, 5, or 7 days of conditioning done by a 12 day old larva. A. CAT=1,12,(7,11,13) B. CAT=3,12,(7,11,13) C. CAT=5,12,(7,11,13) 0. CAT=7,12,(7,11,13) 167 100‘ A 100' B ’1' 90' 90- / i‘ (x " 30' 80‘ / 'U H 'U I O O o 1” 0 m . ’ m J , 70 /__,i . 70' U Y I '2 S I o D . I U c 60 I 1: 60‘ .H .H 8 8 a 50‘ :1 50‘ :11 (U .—1 .4 o\° I o\° 40 40 -———-. 55: CAT=1,12, 7 —-——. 65: CAT=3,12, 7 -_-..155: CAT=1,12,11 ..-....185: CAT=5,12,11 30j .1 112713: CAT=1,12,13 3" .. 5505: CAT=3,12,13 {I_" _ "fitLW 12345678910 12545678910 Time in Days Time in Days 100‘ 100' D I \ 90' / V’ l l 80‘ v F r.. E E .. . _70 p 'U - 6 .2 .5 C1 8 8 >50 >50 8 8 .—J .J °\° 4o- 6940‘ ..__. 95: CAT=5,12, 7 .___. 125; CAT=7,12, 7 .---.. 215: CAT=5,12,11 ......... 245: CAT=7,12,11 3%.: x505: CAT=5,12,13 3‘: x 11565: CAT=7,12,13 CT 7 V V I ' V V ‘U I ' J I I’ V v V I I I I 1 12345678910 12545678910 Time in Days Figure 4.12 Time in Days 168 [comparison 255 versus 34E, Table 4.10, Figure 4.100], and CAT=7,9 (7,13) [comparison 11E versus 35E, Table 4.11, Figure 4.11D]. However, examination of Figures 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12 reveals some apparent age differences in response which the Two—Sample analy- sis is not picking up. For example, in Figure 4.11A curves 2E and 26B appear very different but the Two-Sample procedure says they are the same. Again we have the problem of heterogeneity of variances. When all of these comparisons were rerun with the One-Sample approach (Fl-values in Tables 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12) many more age differences become apparent, and not just at high levels of conditioning. Rather now the trend is a consistent one of age differences in response being at all levels of conditioning, which biologically makes more sense. Comparisons of Figures 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12 reveals that 7 day old larvae consistently respond at a lower level to the same degree of conditioning than 11 or 13 day old larvae and 11 day old larvae are variable, being intermediate between 7 and 13 day old larvae or not different from 13 day old larvae. Figure 4.120 shows the "atypical" groups (12E, 24B, and 36E) as earlier discussed. If these differences are age-related they should disappear when comparing segments of the response vectors where ages are com- patible. For example, comparing the first 4 days of the 13 day old response vector with the last 4 days of the 7 or 11 day old vectors, would result in comparing each test age group from the time the larvae are 13 days old until they are 16 days old. To get some indication if the responses in Tables 4.10, 4.11 and 4.12 are age-related these com- parisons were only made for those groups shown to be different for 169 the first 4 days by the Two-Sample test. The results are shown in Table 4.13. The results in Table 4.13 show that, except for comparison 22E versus 348, the response vectors remain significantly different even for comparable segments of the vectors. It will also be noted that the two significant comparisons with the Two-Sample procedure exhibit non-parallel responses, which means that the larvae may now be exhibiting different behaviors, rather than different degrees of the same behavior. However, we must be very careful about interpreting these results because of the confounding variables, early experience in the colony dishes and length of time in the experimental situation. These will be discussed later. Table 4.13 Results of Hotelling's One (') and Two (*) Sample Tests on comparable 4 day segments of the response vectors shown to exhibit age differ- ences by a Two-Sample comparison in Tables 4.10, 4.11, and 4.12. Comparisons F1 F2 Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. Test Parallelism 10E vs. 34E S.70** H 22E vs. 34E 20.09" 2.79 H P 11E vs. 35E 5.13** H 0 = _ F1(4,76) 2.96 at P - .025. = 5.18 at P = .001. H F2(4,76) HHeterogeneous variances (P_: .05). PMean response vectors are parallel (P §_.OS). = 2.88 at P = .025. = 3.47 at P .001. *F 2(4,155) *5 2(4,155) * 170 Since the groups in Figure 4.2 were run at different times and with larvae from different populations the question arose as to whether behavior is constant over time and from group to group. As a test of this question, replications were periodically run and their mean response vectors compared to the original experimental groups. Nineteen such replications (Figure 4.13) were performed and the results are shown in Table 4.14. The replicates (R) are numbered as their corresponding experimental groups, the only differences being that the replicates were run at some time after the experimental groups and with different larval hatches. In every case, the F1- and F2-values show no difference, but in 13 of 19 comparisons the variances are heterogeneous, which means that behavior is fluctuating from population to population, although the response examined is con- stant. IV. A Brief Analysis Relating to Initial Preference to_Conditioned Food andv the Amount of Larval Movement as Conditioning Increases A. Does Larval Initial Preference to Conditioned Food Change as Degree of Conditioning Increases? It can be seen from examination of Figures 4.4, 4.5, and 4.6 that most initial preferences, i.e., the first point of each response mean vector are probably not good predictors of maximum response to X degrees of conditioning. However, a comparison of initial prefer- ence to low versus high conditioning will give us some idea if dif- ferences do exist. The comparisons made deal with CAT=1,9(7,11,13) vs. CAT=7,9(7,11,13). The analysis is by means of a 2 x 2 contingency 171 .N.v madman a“ cmfimou Hancoswuomxo on» scum museum wouooaom xfisovcmu mo msowumowamom nH.v oasufim mA\NH\N wh\mfl\m «we - p , zmv UV. 2 9 A\HH\NH mA\NH\N «A\m\NH VA\NH\N A\~fi\~fl an. mmm- men «mm- mam mom. raw I, m... mn\mfi\n - - —— .\a mtoin 2:... «5&2 mimic {EH23 I. V5\HN\HH «mm mHN «AH mo“ mm” q.- 1 w w v eh\~H\m un- S M m5\mfl\m Ln .4. Naofl mu\o\v m5\0\¢ ¢A\NH\m <5\V\NH Hmofi an «m an E o o S o S o o a. o o 48.. k m _ 49:91 OZ.ZO_.:DZOU *0 mmumvmo 172 Amonmumu moucmanwfi mfioocomonouozm .o>fim=Hocoo:O mm: osfim>uo O A Q .OO. n O OO ON.N u OO OOO ON.O u OO .ONO. n O OO OO.N uOOOO OOONO. .ONO. n O OOO.N.N "OON OOOOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. OOOOOOO ONO. ONO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NON. OOONO OOOO : OOO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NOO. NON. OOMO .O> .OO.O OOO. ONO. ONO. OOO. NOO. OOO. NOO. NOO. ONO. ONN. OOOON OOO OOO.O OO O OO O NON. ONO. ONO. OOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. NOO. OOO. ONN. OOON. NON. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. OON. OOOOOMO ON ON.O NOO. NON. OON. ONN. NON. OOO. ONN. NON. NON. OOO. ONO .O> ONO.O OO O NON. ONN. ONN. NON. NON. NOO. OON. NON. OON» ONO. ON“. ON OON. OON. OOO. OOO. NOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. OON. OOOOOMO OO OOO.O ONO. ONO. OOO. ONN. ONO. NOO. ONN. NON. ONN. ONO. OON .O> OOm.O, NO O NOO. NON. NOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OON. NON. OON. NOO. OMMI, OO NON. NON. OON. OOO. OON. OON. ONN. ONN. OON. NON. OOOOOMO OO OOO.O OO.O ONO. NOO. ONN. OON. ONN. NON. NOO. NOO. OOO. NOO. OOx .O> OOO.O ONO. NON. NON. NON. NOO. OON. ONO. OOO. ONO. NOO. OON. OO OOOe O NO OO OO O O N O O O O N O OOOOOO Opmom mxmo :O oeflh .goum: cu nova: Scum bum mafia uo>o wcfimcmgo m“ How>mson Hugues: mo pump a mm mcofiumofiOmon Hausa an“: ~.v ohswflm scum mmsopm OmucoEwuomxo nepooOom xOEowamH mo mnemfiummaoo oOmamm mOUose paw n.vo:o m.m:fiHHouoz vH.v oHan 173 .0>Hm3HUr—OUCH v.63 OSHN>IQ . hmo. w Ac Omocmfihm> Ozoocomoyouoxr O A A .OO. n O OO ON.N u OO OOO ON.O u OO .ONO. n O NO OO.N «OOOO OOONOO .ONO. n O OO ON.N "OON OOOOO. OON. OON. NON. OON. NON. NON. OON. OON. OON. OON. OOOOOMO ONO OOO.O ONO. ONO. NOO. NOO. OOO. ONO. NOO. ONO. ONO. NOO. ONOO .O> OOO.O OO O ONO. OOO. NOO. OOO“ OOO. NOO. NOOO.NOO. OOO. NOO. ONMO ONO OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OON. OOOOOMO NNO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. NOOMO NOOO O ONO. ONO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NON. OOOWO .O> OON.O NON. NOO. NOO. OOO. OOO. NOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. NON. NOOOO OOOO OOO.O OO O OO.O OOO. ONO. ONO. OOO. NOO. OOO.MOOO. NOO. ONO. ONO” OOOOM OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NOO. OON. OOOOOOO N NNO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. NOOWO NOOO : OOO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NOO. NON. OONO .O> OON.O NON. NOO. NOO. OOO. OOO. NOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. NON. NOOOO OOO . ON.O ON.O . . . . . . . . . . OOO OOO O , NON ONO ONO OOO NOO ONO NOO NOO OOO ONN O OOOO O NO OO OO O O N O O O O N O OOOONO m .xom 995 Car 25L» O.O.O:ouO OO.O OOOOO . mmo.VOV $2829, Ozoocomoufiozx 174 .o>OO:Oocoo:O mm: osOm>ua u H .mo. u m um OO.N u an vcw nw.H u 40 Q Q .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OOOO OONO. .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OON OOOO. OOO. OOO. ONO. ONO. OOO. NON. OOOOOMO it» ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NON. OOOmO OOON O ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NNN. OOWO .O> OOO.N . ONO. NOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. ONN. OOONm OON OOO.N OO O ON O OOO. ONO. NOO. ONO. NOO. ONO. OOmm1 NOO. NOO. ONO. OOO. ONO. OOO. OOOOOOO N 0 O C . C O “NH NOO ONO ONO OOO ONO OOO NO ONO O NOO. OOO. ONO. NOO. ONO. OOO. ONMO .O> OOO.O OOO. OOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. NON. ONOM ONO OOO.O OO O OO O OOO. NOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. mNmu ONN. ONN. NON. OON. NON. OON. OOOOOOO OOm OOO OOO N ONO. ONO. OON. ONN. NON. NOO. m .O> ONO.O NN O NOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. NON. ONN. OOMO OOO OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. OON. OOOOOMO . . . . . . OOO O NNO OOO NNO NON OOO OON NO OOO OOO. NNO. NON. OON. OON. NON. OOMO .O> ONO.O ONN. NOO. ONN. ONN. NON. NOO. OOO“ OOO mmwOO ON O OO O NON. ONN. ONN. NOO. NOO. ONO. OOww OOOO O NO OO O O O O N O OOOOOO m.xom mxmo cw mags O.O.u:ooO OO.O OOOOO 175 .nmo.W& 3053.2; Osoocomoacpozz .®>Mm3HUCOUCfi WN3 mDHN>IQ N H .mo. " am PM OHoN " DO “:6 MWOH " JO .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OOOO OONOO .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OON OOOO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NOO. NON. OOOOOOO OOO. ONO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OON. ON O O ONO. ONN ONO. OOO. ONO. OOO. ONO. OON. NO .O> ONO.O ONO. NOO. NOO. OOO. NOO. OOO. ONNm ONN OOO.N NO O OO O NOO. ONO. NOOmLmOO. OOO..mOO. ONOO ONO. NOO. NOO. OOO. OOO. NON. OOOOOMO ONO. ONO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OON. NOOMO NOON O ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NON. OOOmO .O> OOO.N ONO. ONO. ONO. NOO. OOO. OON. NOON“ OOON OOO.N OO O OO O ONO. NOO. NOO. ONO. NOO. ONN. OOOOO OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOO. NON. OOOOOOO N ONO. ONO. ONO. OOO. OOO. OON. NOOWO NOON O ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOO. NNN. OOmO .O> OOO.N ONO. ONO. ONO. NOO. OOO. OON. NOO m OON OOO.N OO O OO O OOO. ONO. NOO. ONO. NOO. ONO. OONx OOOO O NO OO O O O O N O OOOOOO O.xom Oxmo 5 2:3. O.O.OOOOO O0.0 OOOOO 176 . 30. W E moocmwhm> msoocomohoumm O .o>OO:Oo:oo:w Om: 03Hm>uo u H .mo. n a an OO.N u no paw mm.O u 4o a a .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OOOO OONO. .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OON OOOO illi— ONO. ONO. OOO. ONO. OOOOOMO ONO. NOO. NOO. OOO. OOWO O OOO OOO ONO. NOO. ONO. ONO. NO .O> NN.N ONO. NOO. NOO. NOO. OOOx OOO OO.N NO O ON N ONO. NOO. ONO. ONO. OOO“ ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOOOOMO ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOMO OOO O ONO. OOO. OOO. ONO. OOMO .O> NO.N ONO. OOO. OOO. OOO. OOOM OOO ON.N OO O ON O ONO. NOO. OOO. OOO. OOmm, NOO. ONO. OOO. OON. OOOOOOO OOO. OOO. NNO. OOO. ONNO O ONN ONN NNO. NNO. OOO. ONN. NO .O> OO.N NOO. NON. ONN. OON. ONNM ONN OO.N OO O OO O ONN. ONN. NON. NOO. ONO“ OON. ONN. ONN. ONN. OOOOOO NO OON . . OON . ON N OOO. ONO. NOO. NOO x O> . .O - ON N NN OOO. OOO. NOO. NOO. mem, OON quH. 9 mm, OO O m N O Omsono O.xom Oman cw cage 0.0.0couO OO.O OOOOO 177 .mmo.WO: Ooocmwnm> Osoocomououomf .o>OO:Ho:oo:O Ow: osfim>ua u H .mo. u a pm OO.N u :9 OOO OO.O u an Q Q .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OOOO OONOO .ONO. n O OO OO.N "OON OOOO. OON. NON. OON. NON. OOOOOOO xwm mwv ON.N NOO. OOO. OOO. ONO. O .O> OOO.O NO O ONO. NOO. NOO. OOO. OOOMI OOO NON. NON. OON. NON. OOOOOOO OOW OOO OOO.N NOO. NOO. NOO. NOO. O .O> NO.N OO O ONO. NOO. ONO. NOO. OOOM OOO NOO. NOO. OOO. ONO. OOOOOMO NOO. NOO. NOO. ONO. OOMO O OOO OOO NOO. NOO. ONO. OOO. NO .O> ON.N NOO. NOO. NOO. OOO. OOOO OOO NO.N OO O NO O NOO. NOO. ONO. ONO. OON“ OOOe O NO OO O O N O OOOOOO m.xom mxmo cw oEOH O.O.OOOO OO.O OOOOO 178 table with a Chi—square analysis and the results are shown in Table 4.15. The results in Table 4.15 indicate that, at least initially, 7 and 11 day old larvae do not discriminate between low and high degrees of conditioning. However, when the total mean response vectors for 2E versus 11B and 14B versus 23B are compared (Tables 4.4 and 4.5) differences in response are detected. Therefore, initial preference is not a good predictor for long-term preference. The 13 day old test larvae, on the other hand, does discriminate in its initial response to low versus high conditioning. There would there- fore appear to be an age difference in the ability to discriminate between varying degrees of conditioning. However, if the initial Table 4.15 Chi-square analysis of initial larval preference to the extremes of conditioning within the three test ages. Age of Test Com arisons Number in Number in Calculated Larvae p Cond. food Plain food Xz-value Low Cond.(2E) 39 41 7 vs. 3.0636 High Cond.(llE) 50 30 Low Cond.(l4E) 51 29 11 vs. 2.9760 High Cond.(23E) 61 19 Low Cond.(26E) 53 27 13 vs. 7.4295** High Cond.(3SE) 7O 10 2 - *X = 3.81 at P = .05. (1) **X2 = 6.635 at P = .01. (1) 179 response of 7 day old larvae to high conditioning versus low con- ditioning (50 versus 30) is compared, by means of a 2 x 2 contingency table with Chi-square analysis, with the initial response of a 13 day old larva to high versus low conditioning (70 versus 10) a Xz-value of 13.33 results. This is significant at the .005 level. Therefore, not only do 13 day old larvae show a higher discriminating between low versus high conditioning but their initial preference to high conditioning is greater than for that of a 7 day old larva. B. Does Amount of Larval Movement Vary in Different Amounts of Conditioning? Experiments up to this point have shown that larvae prefer conditioned food over non-conditioned food and that as conditioning increases the frequency of larvae found in conditioned food increases. This would indicate that larvae prefer to settle in conditioned food and that the number of movements from plain food to conditioned food should increase as conditioning increases. As a partial test of this hypothesis it was determined how many times larvae moved to conditioned food and vice versa, for each of the test ages in a low-conditioned situation (groups 2E, 14E, and 26E) and a highly-conditioned situation (groups 11E, 23B, and 35E). The results are shown in Table 4.16. To equalize number of days spent in the experimental situation, only counts for the first 4 days were used in this analysis. The results in Table 4.16 show that, regardless of age, sig- nificantly more larvae move to the highly conditioned food than move away from it. At low conditioning, however, only 7 day old larvae show a significant difference. Examination of the observed frequencies 180 .mo.o u a um va.m u OOO Nx OOONO.O M MO mewm MW wwwmu OOO OO OONNO.O MUM MO mewm MW wwwwm OON OO OOOO OONNO.O MHWM MN mewm MW mw»mm OOO N OOOO.O MN MW mewm MW wwwmu OON OO NO.N MHMM mm mewm MW wwwwm OOO OO OOO OONNO.O MHWM MN meww MW ww»mm ON N OOOOO. .OOOO .OOOOO OOO.O: .cmO: no 30H nonuflo OO mcficofipflucoo can: mmaaO doom :OOOQ cam wocofluflw -coo coozpos vm>oe om>OmO OOEOO mo Hones: may no OOOxOmcw paw mo OOocwoom mumscm-O:o a mo OOOSOOO O0.0 manmh 181 also shows that, regardless of the degree of conditioning, the number of movements of 7 or 11 day old larvae (younger larvae) are greater than for older 13 day old larvae. For example, in high conditioning, 7 day old larvae moved a total of 29 times in 4 days as opposed to only 14 times for 13 day old larvae. In a chi—square goodness of fit test this yields a XZ-value of 5.2324 which is significant at the 0.025 level. The number of movements by an 11 day old larvae however, is not different from either 7 or 13 day old movements. No differ- ences exist between total movements in low conditioning for any of the three test ages. Control for Food Limitation Before making conclusions about the effects of homotypic con- ditijning on the distribution of isolate larvae, it is first necessary to demonstrate that food is not a limiting factor. To demonstrate thiis, an experiment was run in which developmental time on a maximally conditioned food lump was compared to developmental time on a plain food lump. A six-group experiment was set up with N=20 per group. In three groups each dish had a single food lump conditioned by a 9 day old larva for 7 days. After conditioning the 9 day old larvae were removed and isolate 7, 11, or 13 day old larvae reared until pupation. The Other three groups were identical except the food was not con- ditioned. If developmental time (pupation time) does not differ among Azfifiese 351)< groups, then food was not a limiting factor in my experiments. 141/1e res’ults of an analysis of variance on this data is shown in r’g—ble 4~17 and it can be seen that all pupation times were the same. Tpe means and standard deviations for the 7, 11, and 13 day old 182 Table 4.17 Analysis of variance on developmental times of 7, 11, and 13 day old larvae reared on single food lumps which were either maximally con- ditioned or non-conditioned. Source of Variation d.f. S.S. M.S. F ratio Between Group 5 0.7416 0.1483 - 0.2372 Within Group 114 71.2500 0.625 Total 119 71.9916 F(5’119) = 2.2914 at P = .05. isolates reared on a single unconditioned food lump were 18.35 :_0.55, 18.50 :_0.68, and 18.55 :_0.57, respectively. The means and standard deviations for the 7, 11, and 13 day old isolates reared on a single conditioned food lump were 18.55 :_0.78, 18.60 i 0.56, and 18.50 :_0.57, respectively. V. Can Response to Conditioning Be Overridden by Varying the Food Source? Park (1948) stated that there is probably some kind of inter- action between conditioning and food source involved in the response of Tribolium to conditioned food. With Galleria larvae, I was inter- ested in finding out if larval initial preferences distinguish between conditioned food sources which are either poor in nutrients or rich in nutrients. In order to make such a test, a diet (deficient diet) was needed on which development is delayed. The following diet was used: Pablum = 43.16g, yeast = 0g, honey = 7.5g, glycerin = 31.16g, water = 18.16g, and beeswax = 0g, whereas the normal diet consists of pablum = 32.98g, yeast = 9.7g, honey = 24.25g, glycerin = 21.34g, 183 water = 8.73g, and beeswax = 3g. Sixteen 11 day old larvae were reared on single 4 gram lumps of deficient diet and sixteen on single lumps of normal diet (the normal diet being that used in all previous experiments). The ANOVA in Table 4.18 demonstrates that deveIOpment was slower on the deficient diet, it being 26.12 :_0.52 days on deficient and 18.37 :_0.3 days on normal food. Table 4.18 Analysis of variance on 11 day old larvae reared on normal and deficient diets. Source of Variance d.f. S.S. M.S. F ratio1 Between Group 1 480.5 480.5 168.5964* Within Group 30 85.5 2.85 Total 31 560.00 *F(l,31) = 13.2222 at P = 0.001. To determine if larvae exhibit a preference for normal over deficient food, 16 dishes were set up, each with one normal and one deficient 4-gram food lump on which no conditioning was allowed. One 11 day old larva was tested for preference on each dish. Data was collected for initial preference (the preference 5 minutes after being placed in the experimental situation) and preference 24 hours later. The results were that all 16 larvae preferred the normal food lump at both data points. I next set up an experiment consisting of two groups of 16 dishes each. In one group each dish had two normal food lumps one of 184 which was conditioned. In the other group, each dish had one normal food lump and one deficient conditioned lump. Tests were made with one 11 day old larva per dish and data collected for initial and 24 hour preferences. The data being analyzed is the number of times larvae that initially selected the deficient conditioned lump or the normal conditioned lump switched (flipped) to the normal plain lump in 24 hours. The results are shown in Table 4.19, where DC = deficient-conditioned lump and NC = normal-conditioned lump. Table 4.19 A 2 x 2 contingency table analyzed by chi-square of larval responses to normal-conditioned and deficient-conditioned food. 0c NC Flip 8 0 8 No Flip 2 16 18 10 16 26 Calculated X2 = 18.5287* *x2 = 7.879 at p = 0.005. There is therefore, an interaction between food source and conditioning. Initial preference is to conditioning but food prefer- ences will over—ride the initial preference in the deficient food situation. It should be noted, however, that there is also a dif- ference in initial preference for deficient-conditioned versus normal- conditioned food. Only 10 larvae initially selected the deficient con- ditioned lump and then 8 of the 10 flipped, whereas all 16 larvae selected the normal-conditioned lump with no flips. 185 VI. Do Adult Galleria Exhibit Prefer- ences to Conditioning Done by Larvae? Adult Galleria (metamorphosed) do not eat and are not normally found in the larval food source, and conditioning would probably have very little attraction for them from a feeding point of view. Paddock (1913) and Oertel (1962) both indicate that adult females exhibit pref- erences for oviposition sites and Oertel (1962) further demonstrated that adults are attracted to bee hives which have been larval con- ditioned. I therefore decided to ask if female adults prefer to lay their eggs in conditioned food. Two groups of 20 dishes were set up. In one group each dish had 2, 4-gram normal food lumps, one of which was conditioned by a 9 day old larva for 3 days (low conditioned). In the other group one of the lumps was conditioned by a 9 day old larva for 7 days. The conditioning larvae were then removed and one female adult was placed in each dish. The females were taken from the breeding tank. After 5 days the eggs laid on each food lump were counted and each group analyzed by a chi—square goodness of fit test as shown in Table 4.20. Female adults exhibit no distinction between low-conditioned food and non-conditioned food for egg laying. However, they prefer non-conditioned food to highly conditioned food for egg-laying. Discussion Studies with invertebrates indicate that biological condition- ing affects spatial distribution. These studies, however, deal with whole populations of organisms and are confounded by animal-animal interactions, sexual differences, and various environmental parameters. 186 Table 4.20 Chirsquare goodness of fit test on female adult preference for egg- laying in low and high larval conditioned food. Observed Expected Comparison Number Number Calculated of eggs of eggs X -va1ues Low Cond. Food 5705 5785.5 vs. 1.3980 Plain Food 5832 5768.5 High Cond. Food 3742 5019 vs. 649.8222* Plain Food 6296 5019 *X2 = 7 879 at P = 0 005 (1) . . . No sfiystematic attempt has been made to elucidate the effect of con- ditioning on individual behavior. Park's work (1934, 1935), for example, is confounded by the presence of every developmental stage in his culture dishes, and Naylor‘s (1959) studies were unable to definitively separate conditioning effects from sexual or density factors. Wellington (1957) developed a bioassay for individual behavioral response of the tent caterpillar to light, but not to con- ditioning, and some of the responses of his Type II (less active) larvae could be interpreted in terms of lack of proper stimuli from homotypic conditioning. Surtees (1963a, l964e) completely ignored the effects that biological conditioning may have been having on popu— lations of Tribolium and various grain beetles, and it is not clear from Long's (1955) data if aggregation formation and feeding behavior 187 in butterfly larvae are a function of biological conditioning, animal- animal interactions, or both. The hypothesis for my studies in this paper has been that the spatial distribution of isolate Galleria larvae is a function of larval conditioning and that preference for conditioned medium is a function of the degree of conditioning and age of responding larvae. Larval Responses to Increasing Degrees of Conditioning The between-group comparisons of larval responses to con- ditioning showed that response increases as days of conditioning increase, as age of conditioning larvae increase, or as a function of the two. Older larvae may be conditioning the medium more than younger larvae or they may be differentially conditioning the medium. In every case examined, individual responses to conditioning are either random or attraction. Avoidance was never seen, but a response plateau was indicated. There may be a threshold effect in which con- ditioning beyond a certain level will not elicit a greater response. This requires further investigation particularly in reference to whether conditioning beyond that used in my experiments may elicit avoidance reactions from the test larvae. Such experiments, however, would need to carefully control for avoidance to food limitation. It may be that repulsion to conditioning is not an appropriate behavior 'unless the conditioned medium is also food limited. Park (1948) noted that Tribolium was repelled by excessive conditioning, but his system was feed limited in that the medium used was two year old colony food. 188 Age Differences in Response to Con- ditioningand Possible Mechanisms The data on age differences in response to the same degree of conditioning are not as clear. Significant age differences in response were evident only at the highest degrees of conditioning when the Two- Sample comparison was utilized and at all degrees of conditioning when the One-Sample test was used. There is also every indication (Table 4.13) that these differences do not disappear by comparing identical segments of the mean response vectors. Two-Sample comparisons in Table 4.13 show that not only do age differences not disappear by comparing equal segments of the response curves, but that non—parallelism tends to result as well, indicating the presence of interactions other than age or that behavior is qualitatively different. The age analyses may be con- founded by differences in larval size, activity, sensory capabilities, and early experiences, both in the colony and the experimental apparatus. Size of Responding Larvae The size of different aged larvae may have been a factor in their ability to negotiate the distance between food lumps. As shown in Figure 4.1, the size difference between a 7 and a 13 day old larva is great. The distance between food lumps was 35 mm across the center of the dish. Therefore, 7 day old larvae had a greater relative dis- tance to travel from one food lump to the other than did 13 or 11 day old larvae. If, for example, a 13 day old larva initially chose the plain food lump, it could extend part of its body out of that lump and almost encounter the conditioned lump. A 7 day old larva would 189 have to completely leave the plain food and travel a relatively large distance before encountering the conditioned lump. Activity Differences Galleria larvae may exhibit activity differences dependent upon age. Activity is being defined as amount of movement between food lumps, not within food lumps. Wellington (1957) showed that tent caterpillars are of several types as related to activity levels, some being active and others being sluggish and less investigatory. How- ever, these types are genetically determined not age dependent, although Wellington's data is not very convincing. Long (1955), on the other hand, demonstrated that butterfly larvae exhibit different activity rhythms as development progresses. The older larvae became more active and tended to exhibit many investigatory excursions, and this developmental trend may be common to Lepidoptera. If 7 day old Galleria are initially less active and if response to conditioning is tactual rather than olfactory, they would tend to randomly select a food llUflp and remain there for a time. As they age, however, their activity pattern changes to investigatory and they move out to explore the surroundings. Assuming conditioning to be a preferred stimulus, when they encounter the conditioned food they stop moving and settle. This behavior would account for the low initial preferences of 7 day old test larvae and the gradual initial rise in their response vectors as they find the conditioned food. The more active 13 day old larvae investigate their surroundings immediately. Upon encountering con- ditioned food their preferred stimulus lessens their activity and they settle. This behavior is reflected in the higher initial preferences 190 shown by 13 day old larvae to high degrees of conditioning. Defini- tive experimental support for this hypothesis is lacking, but obser- vational data and the mobility data in section IVA tend to support it. The mobility data shows more total_movements between food lumps by 7 day old larvae than for 13 day old larvae, which indicates that 13 day old larvae find and settle in the conditioned food early, whereas 7 day old larvae do more exploratory movements. However, the crucial point is not total activity of the dif- ferent ages, but when the activity is taking place. If, for example, we re-examine Table 4.16, we find that 7 day old larvae moved a total of 29 times to only 14 times for 13 day old larva. This indicates that 7 day old larvae are more active than 13 day old larvae. However, the data sheets reveal that 7 day old larvae move a total of 6 times in the first two days of the experiment and 13 day old larvae moved a total of 7 times. This means that only 20.68% of the 7 day old movements occurred in the first two days to 50% for 13 day old larvae. Therefore, 7 day old larvae are, at least initially, relatively immobile and are not exploring their environment and finding the con- ditioned lump as soon as are older more active larvae. Even though the total mobility data shows 7 day old larvae to be more active than 13 day old larvae, this activity is occurring 3 or 4 days into the experiment at a time when the initially 7 day old larvae are older and apparently their activity has increased. If this hypothesis is true, we would expect the response ‘vectors for all degrees of conditioning to eventually reach the same level because all larvae will eventually find the conditioned food. 191 This does not happen. When a larva initially selects a plain food lump, it begins conditioning it. When investigatory movements set in, the larva may sample the initially conditioned lump but find that it is not as highly conditioned as the lump it just left and therefore return to its original choice. For example, the initial response of a 7 day old larva to conditioning done by a 9 day old larva for one day is low; that is, many 7 day old larvae initially chose the plain lump. Because their activity is low at this time, they remain in the plain lump and begin conditioning it. When they are older and their activity increases they begin to investigate their surroundings and encounter the initially conditioned lump. However, the conditioning they did for 3 or 4 days is greater than that done by the 9 day old larva for 1 day and they return to the "preferred" stimulus and settle. If the initially conditioned lump had more conditioning than the lump they left they will settle in it. Thus, the mean response vectors for low conditioning remain at a lower level than for high conditioning. These behaviors would necessitate some ability on the part of the larvae to discriminate between different levels of conditioning. Although there are indications of this ability in my studies it needs further testing. Sensory Differences Between Ages There may be sensory differences between ages, in which 13 day old larvaerhave better discriminatory powers for conditioning than do 7 day old larvae. The initial preference analysis in section IVA shows that, at least initially, 7 and 11 day old larvae do not distinguish between high and low conditioning, whereas 13 day old larvae do. 192 Whatever the mechanism(s), response to conditioning is more highly developed in older larvae. The analysis shows that there is a dif- ference in initial preference of 7 or 13 day old larvae to highly conditioned food which tends to support a hypothesis relating to differential sensory ability. However, this initial preference data may also be interpreted in terms of activity differences. The signifi- cant initial preference of 13 day old larvae for conditioned food over plain food may be a reflection of their greater activity and early exploration of the experimental situation, whereas non-significance in 7 day old initial preferences for conditioned over plain food could be a reflection of their relative inactivity once any food lump is encountered. Early Experience Factors Early experience factors may also play a role in the age dif- ferences seen in my experiments. Seven day old test larvae are taken from the 7 day old colony dish which is less highly conditioned than the 11 or 13 day old colony dishes. Therefore, the older the test larva, the more early experience it has had with conditioning and with higher degrees of conditioning. This may account for a 13 day old larva's response to a high degree of conditioning being greater than a 7 day old's, and would be extremely difficult to control. One approach would be to raise larvae, for testing, as isolates and change their food every day, but this would not control for self-conditioning. Even if’the food is changed daily a 13 day old larva would be experiO encing;its own conditioning which is greater than a 7 day old larva's. 193 Another approach would be to raise all larvae in mass cultures and base larval age for experimentation on larval size. Finally, there may be an interaction between age and time spent in the experimental situation, 7 day old larvae spending more time in the dishes than 13 day old larvae. This would be difficult to control and still be able to follow total response mean vectors for each group. However, time spent in the experimental dishes is probably not a factor causing the differences in age. The analysis for age differences was initially with the first four days of the experimental period, regardless of age, and the differences were demonstrated. Response to Biological Conditioning as a Bioassay of Behavior The response of larvae to various degrees of homotypic con- ditioning is a good bioassay of individual behavior. The behavioral responses to graded series of conditioning has been shown to be con- stant for any given age and degree of conditioning. The replications of randomly selected groups in Figure 4.13 and Table 4.14 demonstrate that the response mean vectors do not change over time or from popu- lation to population of larvae, i.e., from hatch to hatch. However, these comparisons show that the variances between populations of larvae are heterogeneous. The behavioral response to conditioning may have become evolutionarily fixed with allowances for variability in how the response is achieved. That is, there are various strategies by which the same response is achieved. 194 If the response itself is of major interest, as it was in my studies, and not the variability in response from population to population, then the heterogeneous variances indicate the desirability of selectively breeding a "standard" Galleria stock for future experi- mentation. Response to conditioning could be used as a bioassay of the behavior being selected for. As repeatedly mentioned and demonstrated throughout these Studies the variances are heterogeneous. This has profound conse- quences on the power of the test to discriminate between a true and a false null hypothesis. Coupled with the positive serial correlation in my data two responses would have to be very different before being detected with the Two-Sample test, even with such large sample sizes. The power of the test was noticeably increased when the One-Sample test was employed, mainly because this procedure utilized the lower covariance matrix as the "best estimate" of the variances. Utilizing both procedures has enabled some insight into the behavior or Galleria larvae in responding to biological conditioning. Aside from being heterogeneous, the variance vectors in Table 4.3 show a trend of decreasing magnitude as degree of condition- ing increases. In other words, as conditioning increases the behavior of the larva tends to become more fixed. They select the conditioned food lump and show very little variation in response. If, however, conditioning is low the response is variable, with a good deal of movement between the conditioned and plain food. One might think of :response in a low-conditioned situation as an equilibrium state in vflrich there is a great deal of oscillation between food lumps, but 195 about a predictable level. As conditioning increases, however, these oscillations decrease in number as more and more larvae select the conditioned food lump and remain there. This trend in decreasing variance is also seen in the response of the 3 test ages to the same degree of conditioning. As larvae get older they tend to select and stay in conditioned food. This is probably related to the mobility differences discussed earlier. There are any number of sources for variation in such experi- ments. Some of it may be due to environmental variability. Although light, temperature, and humidity were closely controlled there may have been undetected differences from experiment to experiment. However, these differences were not great enough to affect the results of larval selection as indicated by the replication data in Table 4.14. Genetic variability may be the main contributor to the hetero- geneous variances. Many experiments were run with different popu- lations of larvae. Although controls and replications indicate this did not affect the behavior being looked at, the genetics may have ‘been changing enough to account for the variability. This is impos- sible to determine at this time since the genetics of the larvae was ‘not closely regulated or monitored. An interesting fact from all my tables, however, is that even ‘though the variance of any two groups is heterogeneous, the response (nirves are parallel. Parallelism indicates that, although at different levels, the response curves were achieved by similar behaviors. The heterogeneity may be a result of the degree to which each behavior is exhibited under varying degrees of conditioning. In selecting low or 196 high conditioned food, a larva utilizes its preference behavior for conditioning but this behavior increases and is less fluctuating as conditioning increases. Possible Functions of Biological Conditioning My studies have demonstrated that the spatial location of Galleria larvae may be largely determined by their homotypic con- ditioning. Individual larvae are attracted to conditioning and this attraction is a function of both the degree of conditioning and age of the larvae. These studies will be pursued further in the next chapter in relation to animal-animal interactions and resulting effects on individual preferences to conditioning. Aside from its function in spatial distribution, biological conditioning may be important for other reasons. Some possibilities are that conditioning (1) serves as protection for the larvae, (2) pro- ‘vides a preferred consistency of the medium, (3) facilitates mobility, (4) signals the presence of other larvae, and (5) is related to feed- ing behavior. Protection Paddock (1913) hypothesized that spinning behavior in Galleria larvae serves as a scaffolding to prevent the hive combs from collap- siJu; as the larvae tunnel throughout the hive and literally decimate it. Although this is an interesting hypothesis, there is no support for it. Paddock (1913) also postulated that silk tunnels protect the larvae from the bees. The only support for this hypothesis is circum- stantial. Free (1961) noted that bees sting dark colors, rough 197 surfaces, and rapidly moving objects. Galleria larvae, particularly older larvae, tend to be darkly colored and move in rapid, jerky movements. In their whitish, smooth, silken tunnels, however, their color and movements may go undetected. However, the interior of a bee hive is dark and vision on the part of the bees may not be utilized in detecting Galleria. A possible test for both these hypotheses would be to arti- ficially select for a population of Galleria that do not spin these tunnels. Comparisons could then be made to see if any selective advantage accrues to the spinning population. Biological Conditioning as a Preferred Consistency Larvae may prefer the consistency or the "feel" of biologically conditioned medium over non-conditioned medium. Although not a very tenable hypothesis, it does have minor support. Beck (1960) found that larvae preferred certain medium over others. Since he could not find any adaptive advantage to such a preference, he concluded that larvae like the consistency. Other workers, however, found a better explanation. Young (1961) found that the preferred medium enhanced larval growth, and Chase (1921) earlier noted that larvae raised on poor diets transform into smaller and less viable adults. However, consistency may well be a factor in their preferences if, for example, medjinn is easier to tunnel through. Mobility Mobility may be easier or quicker in conditioned than in non- conditiomed food. This may be related to the silk itself or to the 198 fact that the tunnels provide ready access through the food. Stanley (1949), for example, demonstrated with Tribolium that tunnel-making in the food enhances mobility and that the beetles preferred using existing tunnels rather than constructing new ones. A Signal for the Presence of Other-Larvae Biological conditioning may indicate the presence of other larvae. If individual larvae are responding to conditioning because it signals the presence of other larvae it would first be necessary to demonstrate that a larva is at least willing to tolerate the presence of other larvae. If it is not, then one would predict an avoidance to conditioning rather than an attraction. A partial test of this hypoth- esis is provided in Chapter V. Biological Conditioning and Feeding Behavior A final, and more probable, possibility is that preference for biological conditioning is related to feeding behavior. Brindley (1910) and Edwards (1910) demonstrated a relationship between silk threads spun by the processionary caterpillars and feeding excursions. Wellington (1957) showed that even relatively inactive larvae will follow silk threads to a food source, and Rathcke and Poole (1975) found that butterfly larvae (Mechantis isthmis) cooperate to spin a silk scaffolding over their food supply, enabling exploitation of an otherwise unavailable food source. Rathcke and Poole further postu- lated that such a massive silk gallery is energetically feasible only if several larvae pool their resources and share the benefits. Most 199 larvae spin some silk even if only during pupation and the evolution of a feeding web would entail only an elaboration of an already present ability. However, such behavior would require cooperation and possibly some form of social behavior. Long (1955) also demon— strated a relationship between biological conditioning and feeding behavior in larvae of the large white butterfly. Newly hatched larvae initially locate a food supply by following silk trails. Larvae also feed in masses, constantly spinning and enlarging a silk mat around the food supply. Biological conditioning may signal an exploitable food supply to Galleria larvae. The higher the degree of conditioning means the better the food source, and so the greater the response to it, since the food supply would have to be nutritious enough for a larva to spend enough time on it to highly condition it. However, very highly conditioned food might also indicate a food depleted situation which might not affect larval initial preference for the conditioning. Haydak (1936), for example, showed that Galleria larvae reared on deficient diets produce less silk than those reared on normal diets. In my experiments with deficient diets, I had to use more larvae to condition a deficient food lump to the same degree as one larva's cxnuiitioning on normal food, as measured by response of a test larva. These experiments also show that, although initial preference is to cxnuiitioning, if the conditioned food source is deficient the larvae quickly move to a better non-conditioned source. 200 Biological Conditioning and Population Density Biological conditioning may function as an index of larval population density. If this is true, then at some high degree of conditioning, larvae will avoid the conditioned situation. This was not observed in my experiments, probably because conditioning was not high enough, or food not limited as a result of the conditioning. However, a non-preference for highly conditioned food was seen on the part of adults to conditioning. In 1959, Naylor stated that gravid female Tribolium tend to select, for egg laying, niche units of low adult and larval occupancy or avoid those of high occupancy. Chiang and Hodson (1950) had earlier demonstrated that larval Drosophila melanogaster inhibit oviposition of the adults and that oviposition is density-dependent on the larvae. My studies with egg laying preferences of female Galleria show that they avoid highly larval conditioned food for ovipositing, but exhibit a random response if food is only slightly conditioned. The biological conditioning may be functioning as an index of population density, and the survival value of the adult females' behavioral response is obvious. By laying her eggs in low or non-conditioned niches she ensures that hatching larvae will not be in extreme com- petition for the available resources which enhances their chances of survival to breeding age. CHAPTER V THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BIOLOGICAL CONDITIONING AND ANIMAL-ANIMAL INTERACTION IN THE SPATIAL DIS- TRIBUTION OF GALLERIA MELLONELLA (L.) LARVAE Introduction In Chapter IV it was demonstrated that isolate Galleria larvae exhibit a preference for biologically conditioned food, that this preference increases as the degree of conditioning increases, and that possibly an age variable enters into the selection of conditioning. However, biological conditioning is a function of the organisms them- selves and, as such, may be closely linked with interactions between ,other conspecifics. It is the purpose of this paper to look at how animal-animal interactions affect the preferences for conditioning established in Chapter IV. Animal-animal interactions are known to affect the spatial distribution of organisms. Surtees (1963a, b, and c; 1964a, b, c, e and f) found in grain weevils that as density increased, the number lof weevils appearing on the surface of the medium increased, but he failed to account for the extra conditioning as a result of increased densityx A similar oversight was made by Legay and Chase (1964) who ‘hypothesized that dispersal in Tribolium is dependent on exploration and population pressure due to increasing density. They attempted to explain their results purely on the basis of animal-animal interactions. 201 202 For example, they found the mean duration of stay of individual beetles in a partial enclosure to increase with increasing density. They interpret this as a result of accommodation to tactile stimuli among the beetles which had not discovered the way out of the enclo- sure. However, as density rises so does conditioning, and the "accommodation" to tactile stimuli may have been an increasing pref— erence for increasing degrees of conditioning. Naylor (1959 and 1965) found that female Tribolium confusum usually exhibit a uniform distribution but will form very loose aggre- gations in conditioning. Males on the other hand, aggregate in con- ditioning and with other males. His general conclusion was that spatial distribution in Tribolium is dependent upon density, but if sex and conditioning are added as variables the spatial patterns become altered. In 1963, Ghent contrasted the behavioral responses of Tribolium confusum and I, castaneum in mixed populations and demon- strated that the distributions of these populations depend upon animal- animal interactions, response to homotypic and heterotypic conditioning, and differences in behavior between the sexes. Much of the work with Tribolium and other beetle populations is somewhat confounded by density and the effects of sexual behavior (”1 response to biological conditioning. The effects of biological conditioning and animal-animal interactions may be more clearly seen 511 larval populations free from adult—young and sexual interactions. Galleria (larva) is such a model but has not yet been worked out. Edwards (1910) showed that larvae of the processionary caterpillar stay in feeding excursion lines as a result of silk strands and 203 head-to—tail larval contact, the latter being of predominant impor- tance. Several larval forms of butterflies exhibit a relationship between biological conditioning and animal-animal interactions (Long 1955). Silk threads are used to find food but larval—larval inter- actions serve to integrate the cyclic feeding behavior. Long also discovered that varying activity rhythms of different aged larvae affect the ability of individuals to join pre-established feeding groups. Similar kinds of behavior are noted by Wellington (1957 and 1960) with tent caterpillars. Different larval types can be iden- tified on the basis of activity. Type I larvae are capable of inde— pendent, oriented movements, whereas Type II larvae need a silk trail or animal-animal contact for directed movements. Movements are even more directed if both stimuli are present. Although demonstrated that biological conditioning and animal- animal interactions affect spatial patterns, the two variables are often confounded at the population level since there is little if any control over the amount of conditioning populations are doing. There have been only minor attempts at elucidating the effects of these 'variables on individual behavior. Knowing the relationship between individual preference for conditioning and degrees of conditioning (Chapter IV), it is the purpose of this paper to investigate whether these individual preferences are altered by the presence of another conspecific in the larvae of Galleria mellonella. The general hypoth- esis being tested is that individual preference for conditioning is unaffected by the presence of a conspecific and the resulting response 204 is a function of whether the conspecific is a resident in the con- ditioned food or introduced with the test larva. Materials and Methods Husbandry All experimental larvae were reared as described in Chapter II and drawn from colony dishes maintained in darkness in incubators at 32 :_1°C and 75% R.H. The experimental larvae used in these experi- ments are pictured in Figure 4.1 and their weights given in Table 4.1. Apparatus The experimental apparatus consisted of 100 x 25 mm plastic petri dishes with two 4 :_0.05 gram food lumps in each dish. Within any one dish the food lumps were placed on opposite sides of the dish so that test larvae could be introduced between them and allowed to make a choice. Experimental Procedures The experimental procedures for all experiments in this paper arerthe same as described for the experiments in Chapter IV, with respect to how food is prepared and put in the experimental dishes, Inna conditioning is done and how test larvae are introduced and data (Hallected. The only difference between the two procedures is that in these experiments two test larvae are added to each dish rather than one. These test larvae are of varying ages depending on the test sijniation. Both larvae were placed at the center of the dish and allowed to choose a food lump. Data was then collected on the 205 distribution of both larvae over time, although, in some situations, only the response of one of the larvae was monitored. There are several different experimental designs in this paper and each will be described at the proper time. However, there are some nomenclature differences from Chapter IV. Only two different degrees of conditioning are utilized in these experiments, namely CAT=1,12,(7,11,13) and CAT=7,6,(7,ll,13) which will be referred to as LOW and HIGH conditioning respectively, where C = days of conditioning, A = age of conditioning larvae, and T = age of test larvae. Each experimental group has been assigned an identification code for ease in referring to the various comparisons I will be making. For example, codes such as SOP(l-3)7+7 or 64PH(l-S)7+13 will be fre- quently encountered. The first two numbers (50 or 64) are identifi- cation numbers for each experimental group and the rest of the nomen- clature conveys the experiment that was performed. The P stands for "pairs" of larvae, meaning that two test larvae were used in the experiment, whereas an I stands for "isolate" in experiments when a single test larva was utilized (i.e., 50P(l-3)7+7 or 591(l)7). The letter following the P or the I will be either an L or sun H referring to "low" and "high" degrees of conditioning. If :neither an H or I appears in the identification number this means that when the experimental group was tested, at least initially, neither of the food lumps had been previously conditioned (50P(l—3)7+7). The two numbers on the end of the identification number (7+7 or 7+13) iJuiicate the ages of the test larvae at the start of the experiment. 206 The numbers in parentheses refer to how the data for each experimental group is being analyzed. When two larvae are used in the experiments there are five possible ways in which to analyze the data: 1. total number of organisms on the conditioned food lump, regardless of whether paired or single; 2. total number of pairs on the conditioned food lump only; 3. total number of pairs regardless of the food lump on which they occur; 4. the number of older larvae of the pair of test larvae on the conditioned food lump, i.e., response of 13 day old larva when a 7 day old larva is also present; 5. the number of younger larvae of the pair of test larvae on the conditioned food lump, i.e., response of 7 day old larvae when a 13 day old larva is also present. Larvae used in these experiments were not marked for identi- fication purposes. Because I used such large sample sizes, marking them would have been prohibitive, and I would have had to mark them several times during the course of the experiment since they shed their cuticles several times throughout development. This would have :necessitated disturbing the experimental procedures to remark them. 'Therefore, when two test larvae are the same age (i.e., 7+7, ll+ll, or 13»13), individuals cannot be identified. Because of the size (distribution of larvae, however, if two larvae are of different ages (i.e., 7+ll, 7+l3, or ll+13) individuals can easily be identified for analysis. Therefore, when both larvae are the same age, only the 207 first three of the above analyses are possible, whereas when the larvae are different ages all five of the analyses are possible. In one experiment in this paper, neither food lump was pre- conditioned at the start of the experiment. In such a case the five possible analyses do not refer to the conditioned lump. However, one of the food lumps received an identification mark so that it could be distinguished from the other lump, making the five analyses possible as well. The following are a few examples of how to interpret the identification numbers in this paper: SOP(l-3)7+7: Experimental group #50, with a pair of test larvae with no initial conditioning, and in which the ages of the two larvae were 7+7. The possible analyses are (1—3) since both larvae are the same age. 6OI(1)lS : Experimental group #60 with an isolate test larva of 13 days of age. When an isolate is used only the first analysis is ever utilized (1). S7PL(4)7+13: Experimental group #57 with a pair of test larvae tested with Low conditioning. The larvae are 7 and 13 days old and the analysis is on the 13 day old larva in conditioned food (4). 64PH(5)7+13: Experimental group #64 with a pair of test larvae that are 7 and 13 days old and High conditioning was employed. Analysis is on the response of the 7 day old larva to conditioned food (5). 68PH(S)7+1§; Experimental group #68 with a pair of test larvae that are 7 and 13 days old and High conditioning was employEd. The fact that the l§_is underlined indicates that this was a resident larva in the conditioned food and not introduced when the 7 day old test larva was. If neither age is underlined then both larvae were introduced together. Analysis is on the response of the 7 day old larva to conditioned food (5). 208 When two test larvae are of different ages, the length of the mean response vectors is dictated by the age of the older larva, the experiments being terminated when the older larva reaches its wander- ing phase. For example, if the two larvae are 7+13 days of age, only four experimental days will be analyzed. After 4 days the 13 day old larva enters the wandering phase. The 7 day old larva will not enter the wandering phase until 5 days after the 13 day old larva. However, I was interested only in animal-animal interactions during the "larval phase." Therefore, when the older larva enters the wandering phase, data analysis ends. There is one exception to this procedure which will be noted at the proper time. Analysis The dependent variable chosen for analysis within each experi- mental group is the mean response to conditioned food over time for pairs or either member of the pair of test larvae. When conditioned food was not employed, the dependent variable becomes the number of pairs, regardless of food lump, over time. Each experimental group has 80 dishes and, in most cases, each «dish has two test larvae. If both larvae are in the conditioned lump :1 score of 2 is recorded, if neither is in that lump a O is recorded. If only one larva is in the conditioned food a l is recorded, and in the case of different aged test larvae the l is identified as to whixfli larva it represents. It was desirable to be able to compare two- larvae situations with isolate situations. To do this the sample sizes had to be adjusted. The following procedure was used. 209 If, for a two-larval test situation, it was desirable to analyze the number of pairs or the total number of larvae on a particular food lump in all 80 dishes over time, then the matrices would have entries of all two's or zeros for the paired situation and two's, one's, and zeros for total larvae on a particular food lump. These matrices were divided by the number of test larvae per dish, in this case 2, which reduces the entries to either one's, 0.5'5, or zeros. These matrices are now comparable to matrices generated from only one test larva and the sample size is always 80 (the number of dishes) and not 160 and 80. If, on the other hand, the response of only one of the two test larvae is analyzed, the entries in its data matrix are already one's and zeros and need not be divided by 2. Statistical analysis is by means of Hotelling's One- and Two— Sample procedures, as discussed in Chapters III and IV, although some Chi-square and ANOVA analyses are employed. All hypotheses are two- tailed and it was a_priori determined to use an a-level of .025 because of heterogeneous variances, positive serial correlation, and multiple comparisons. These considerations were all discussed in Chapter IV. Results The following description of the results has been divided into tflrree experiments, all of which relate to whether individual preference ier biological conditioning is altered by the presence of a conspecific. Experiment I: This experiment examines the question of whether, in the absence of initial conditioning, Galleria larvae aggregate and whether their distribution is a function of the age of the other member of the pair. 210 Experiment 11: This experiment examines individual preferences for a conditioned food lump when a conspecific is also present and asks whether preference is a function of the conspecific, the age of the conspecific, or whether the conspecific is a resident in the conditioned food. Experiment III: Experiment 11 will demonstrate that when 7+13 day old larvae are tested with high conditioning the 7 day old larva's response to conditioning is significantly lowered initially but increases with time. Experiment III deals with several test relating to whether this behavior is related to the behavior of the 13 day old larva, the conditioning, or an interaction between the two. Experiment I: Do Galleria Larvae Group in the Absence of Initial Conditioning and Is their Distribution in Such a Situation a Function of the Presence of a Conspecific? Before asking whether larval preferences for conditioning are affected by animal-animal interactions, it was decided to determine if larval-larval interactions affect their distribution when neither food lump is previously conditioned. The only valid measure to answer this question is the initial preference (5 minutes after the start of the experiment) of the larvae for being with a conspecific of varying ages or of remaining isolated. The larvae are constantly conditioning their environment so that very shortly after the experiment begins the food becomes conditioned and it would no longer be possible to deter- lnine if response is to the conditioning or the other larva. However, tn! looking at certain facets of the whole experiment, we can get some .Ldea.of which variable may be responsible for the resultant distrib— utions. The experimental design is shown in Figure 5.1. The six experimental groups are shown along with replications (R) of those AGE OF TEST LARVAE AGE OF TEST LARVAE 211 7 11 13 SOP(1-3)7+7 9/11/74 50R(l—3)7+7 4/17/75 SIPCI15)7+11 9/11/74 SlR(1-5)7+11 4/17/75 52P(1-S]7+13 9/11/74 52R(1-S)7+13 4/17/75 539(1—3)11+11 549(1—5)11+13 9/11/74 9/11/74 11 53R(l-3)ll+ll S4R(l-5)ll+13 4/17/75 4/17/75 55P(1-3)13+l3 9/11/74 13 SSR(l-3)l3+l3 4/17/75 Figure 5.1 Experimental design for asking whether larvae will group (pair) in the absence of homotypic conditioning. 212 groups and the dates when each was run. All experimental groups were run at the same time and all replications were run together, but several months later. Each identification code indicates the larval combinations and the possible analysis and each group has a sample size of 80. The question being asked is whether larvae will group (pair) in the absence of initial conditioning and when the age combinations of pairs is varied. The analyses utilized are in relation to pairing regardless of food lump, since both food lumps were "equal" at the start of the experiment. Table 5.1 shows the results of comparing each experimental group with its replication by both the One- and Two-Sample procedures. The results indicate that, although variances are heterogeneous, pairing behavior in the absence of initial conditioning does not change from population to population of larvae. Figure 5.2 (A+B) shows the mean response vectors for percent pairs of larvae on both food lumps when the food lumps were initially unconditioned. The only valid comparison for whether larvae prefer to pair in the absence of conditioning is for the first points on these curves. The results of this comparison may be found as Chi- square values in the first boxes of each comparison in Table 5.5. 'This table will be explained shortly, but for now the important point is that regardless of age combinations, pairs of larvae initially dis- 'tribute themselves randomly. This indicates that, at least initially, the larvae are not reacting to each other. If such reactions were 213 .mo. u a 3m ©3.~ u 3cm.ovaa ecu mm.3 u 30w.oe3o .mmo. u a pm 3m.N u 3mm3.ov~a. use mm.~ u 3vn.oV3a. .mo. n a 3m w~.N «3om.O3U=a an“ 35.3 "30w.O3U3n .mwo. u a pa m3.~ n3me3.03vma cam «N.N "3on.03e3m $3. 3m. «8. SN. 33. SN. #3338? L mo3. om3. New. Now. m3m. NmN. mmmm : mom. m3m. mom. ONN. M33. mmm. ammm 33+33mmwmmm 3cm.3 Ame. Ame. mNA. mmn. Awe. New. «emu 33+333mvamm A3.N ao.o wN.3 N35. con. man. Noe. con. mmm. ammm NV3. aw3. ~3N. 0mm. omm. mew. eo3oomm em3. N03. “om. 0mm. sew. mew. m3mm : @33. n33. @33. 0mm. 3mm. New. a3mm 33+Ammwm3m 330.3 ~3w. cow. N35. cmo. mom. New. 13mm 33+A3mva3m 3mw.3 mm.o mm.3 0mm. cos. “we. owe. omm. mum. a3mm wmm. mNN. "m3. ONN. mam. new. omN. mmN. mmm. omN. eo3oomm emm. oem. m3m. ovw. new. new. mmm. mmm. mmm. mmu. momm m mam. NNN. eww. eww. mam. new. mmN. mmm. mmm. new. aomm A+Ammwmom .om.3 omo. Ame. N30. ewe. ~30. mhm. mam. ~3m. mNm. N3m. «emu n+53mvaom .m¢.3 Nw.o 33.3 5mm. who. ene. Ame. Awm. mum. Ame. mne. mew. mam. aomm ones a «a 3a 03 m m A o m e m m 3 masons m.xom mama cw osfie .mnfiom mo hopes: Hope» we mfimon one so owes one meanwhomaou .cfi ouo: xonp mesa poem coax: mo mmofiuhowon .3.m onsmfim scum anm:0wuoUAHmoh oz» msmuo> Amummsonw Houcoafinomxo mo mcomwuomaou oagaom mawdze van n.uo:o m.m:«33ouo: mo muHSmoz 1m 93.3. 214 A .mo. u a 3m 03.3 n so was mm.3 u a .mNO. n a we mm.~ «3mm3.vVNal ecu om.~u3oe.v33a. mam. mmm. mmm. mmm. eu3ooam mew. 3mm. nmm. mmN. mmmm .: mmm. mmm. mmm. mmm. ammo m3+m3mmwmmm 3m.m Awm. omm. mmm. mmm. ammm M3+m33mVamm om.~ oo.o me.~ com. com. 5mm. oom. ammm w3N. mmN. ANN. mmm. em3ooam om3. ONN. w3N. mmN. memm : mam. mam. 5mm. mmm. avmm m3+33mmwmem 3m.N owe. Noe. Ame. ~3m. mvmm m3+333mvaem mm.~ NN.3 en.~ 5mm. New. mNo. Awe. new“ mMN. mmm. meN. mmm. eo3ooam m3m. GNN. 5mm. mmm. ammm : 3mm. 3mm. mmN. mmm. ammm M3+Ammwmmm A3.N “me. New. mmo. N3m. mmmu m3+n3mvawm m3.~ em.o 3o.N 5mm. 0mm. N3m. mew. ammm amok. a N a 3 m e n N H mmzopu m Fxob who: :3 anr 3.e.3coov 3.m 033me 215 taking place we would expect either significant attraction or repul- sion, when in fact, a random distribution is exhibited. If we examine Figure 5.2 (A G B), however, there is a trend for increased pairing with time. Table 5.2 shows the results of Hotelling's One-Sample comparison of each of these preference curves with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. The F-values show that in only three groups does aggregation become significantly different from random over time, namely in the 7+7, 7+ll, and ll+ll situations. Whenever a 13 day old larva was a member of the pair grouping never reaches significance. This may be an indication that some sort of animal-animal interaction occurs over time. However, the preference vectors in Figure 5.2 and compared in Table 5.2 are unequal in time. Therefore, an analysis was performed using the first four days and the last four days of the experimental periods, the two being the same for 13 day old response vectors. The results of this analysis are presented in Table 5.3. It can be seen that the same groups show significant pairing except that the 7+7 group does not show significant pairing until the end of the experimental period, that is until the larvae have been in the experimental situ- ation for at least four days. One might contend that these differences are a function of time in the experimental situation. That is, 13 day old larvae do not show grouping behavior because they only have four days in the experimental apparatus, whereas a 7 day old larva has 10 days, and an 11 day old larva has 6 days. This may be true of the 7+7 situation, but it will be noted that 7+ll and 11+ll groups significantly pair 216 Figure 5.2 Mean response vectors for percent pairs of larvae on any food lump when the food lumps were initially unconditioned. A. Pairs composed of larvae of the same age. B. Pairs composed of larvae of different ages. 217 A.m m3:m3a m < whom a“ oefib mxoo :fi oeflb 03 m w A o m e m A 03 m m A o m e m A 3 Dr I D D D P P P I D O P b P r P P P P DI L l O H H 2.333333 3. x 4.0... 2.725333 x a 3.0m m3+AEANm r.....l 33+33$Emm 3..-... 33+Afimva3m cuuull. A+A3mvaom .unnun. cow 0/0 IO? .d 9 T... .om m .om O u .oo .m .00 .d m .OA 9. .0A A... m .ow .ow .om .om .o03 30o3 dmnq pood fine no sated % 218 Table 5.2 Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of each ex- perimental (P) group in Figure 5.1 with an expected vec- tor whose entries are all 0.5 to see if grouping (pair- ing), regardless of food lump, is different from ran- dom in the absence of homotypic conditioning. Response mean vectors(X), estimated variance vectors(Sz), and D- values for serial correlation are found in Table 5.1. Groups F 509(3)7+7 2.81** 519(3)7+11 13.32*** 529(3)7+13 0.57 539(3)11+11 4.19** 54P(3)ll+13 2.01 559(3313+13 0.20 **F(10,70)=2.59, F(6,74)=3.08, F(4,76)=3.59 at P=.Ol. ***F(10,70)=3.48, F(6,74)=4.26, F(4,76)=5.18 at P=.001. 219 Table 5.3 Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of pairs, re- gardless of food lump, versus an expected vector whose en- tries are all 0.5. These comparisons are shown for the first four days and the last four days of each experimental group to see if time in the experimental situation affects grouping behavior. First 4 Days Last 4 Days Groups F F SOP(3)7+7 1.07 2.99* 51P(3)7+ll 3.53* 19.64*** 52P(3)7+13 0.57 0.57 53P(3)ll+ll S.83*** 6.02*** S4P(3)ll+l3 2.01 2.01 55P(3)13+13 0.20 0.20 * F(4,76) = 2.96 at P - .025. *** = 5.18 at P = .001. IV4.76) 220 when just the first four days are considered which is the same length of time 13 day old larvae have. In Table 5.4 are presented the results of Hotelling's One- and Two-Sample comparisons for all possible comparisons of the experi- mental groups in Figure 5.1 for the first and last four days of the experimental periods. The F2-values show only one significant dif— ference for the first four days, namely that ll+ll pairs are more frequently found than are 7+7 pairs. For the last four days, however, there are many more differences, the greatest pairing occurring when an 11 day old larva is a member of the pair, and the greatest differ- ence is the 7+ll versus the l3+l3 situation. Again illustrated is the fact that pairing is not significant when at least one member of the pair is a 13 day old larva. The Fl-values in Table 5.4 point out some other differences, probably as a result of the heterogeneous variances in the Two-Sample case. There are four possible ways to explain why aggregation may be occurring over time: (1) there may have been discrepancies between the two plain food lumps due to handling or nutritional differences, (2) the larvae may be responding to conditioning of other larvae, (3) some sort of larval-larval interaction may develop, and (4) com- binations of 1-3 are possible. We have already seen that initial preference for the food lumps is random, indication that the food lumps were equal in all respects. However, if the food lumps are actually not different, then pairing with respect to one food lump should be random, since the larval distribution should not be a function of food. To determine 221 Table 5.4 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons among the experimental groups in Figure 5.1. Comparisons are made for the first 4 days and the last 4 days to see if time in the experimental situation affects grouping, and all comparisons are for pairs of larvae regard- less of food lump. For the One Sample(') Test the groups used for the expected vectors are in the column headed BV. and those whose covariance matrix was used are in the column headed CV. FIRST FOUR DAYS comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 50P(3)7+7 vs. 51P(3)7+ll 3.76" 1.94 H P 52P(#)7+13 vs. 50P(3)7+7 2.65 1.14 H P 50P(3)7+7 vs. 53P(3)ll+ll 3.67** H 50P(3)7+7 vs. S4P(3)ll+13 4.17" 2.22 H P 50P(3)7+7 vs. 55P(3)13+13 0.69 P 52P(3)7+13 vs. SlP(3)7+ll 2.71 1.42 H P 51P(3)7+ll vs. 53P(3)ll+ll 1.66 P 51P(3)7+ll vs. S4P(3)ll+13 2.23 P 55P(3)13+l3 vs. SlP(3)7+ll 4.06" 2.09 H P 52P(3)7+13 vs. 53P(3)ll+ll 5.34"' 2.23 H P 52P(3)7+l3 vs. 54P(3)ll+l3 1.33 P 52P(3)7+l3 vs. 55P(3)13+l3 0.66 0.45 H P 54P(3)11+13 vs. 53P(3)11+11 1.95 0.84 H P 53P(3)ll+ll vs. 55P(3)13+l3 2.44 P SSP(3)13+13 vs. S4P(3)ll+13 1.05 0.49 H P ' F1(4,76)= 2.96 and * F2(4,155)= 2.88 at P = .025. v. ** F1(4,76)= 3.59 and F2(4’155)= 3.47 at P = .01. 111 *** F1(4,76)=‘5’18 and F2(4,155)= 4.94 at P = .001. HHeterogeneous variances (P 5.05). PMean response vectors are parallel. 222 Table 5.4 (cont'd.) LAST FOUR DAYS comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 50P(3)7+7 vs. SlP(3)7+11 5.97*** H 52P(3)7+13 vs. 50P(3)7+7 1.23 P 50P(3)7+7 vs. 53P(3)ll+ll 4.47" 1.50 H P 50P(3)7+7 vs. S4P(3)1l+13 2.07 P 55P(3)l3+l3 vs. 50P(3)7+7 3.41" 1.86 H P 52P(3)7+13 vs. 51P(3)7+ll 5.93*** H P SlP(3)7+ll vs. 53P(3)ll+ll 2.91* H P SlP(3)7+1l vs. 54P(3)11+13 5.50*** H 55P(3)13+13 vs. 51P(3)7+ll 7.87*** H 52P(3)7+13 vs. 53P(3)ll+ll 4.59" 2.34 H P 52P(3)7+13 vs. 54P(3)ll+13 1.33 P 55P(3)l3+13 vs. 52P(3)7+13 0.66 0.45 H P 53P(3)ll+ll vs. 54P(3)ll+13 3.10' 1.39 H P 55P(3)13+13 vs. 53P(3)ll+1l 4.82" 2.16 H P 55P(3)13+13 vs. S4P(3)ll+13 1.05 0.49 H P I * F1(4,76)= 2.96 and F2(4,155)= 2.88 at P = .025. c. ** F1(4,76)= 3.59 and F2(4,155)= 3.47 at P = .01. "'F1(4,76)= 5.18 and ***F2(4,155)= 4.94 at p = .001. HHeterogeneous variances (PfE.05). PMean response vectors are parallel. 223 if larvae are pairing with respect to only food several contingency tables with Chi-square analyses were set up in which pairs on one food lump are compared with pairs on the other lump at each time point as well as for the total experimental period. This enables parcelling out the contribution of each time point in respect to pairing. If significance at any one or more time points results or if the Chi- square values are significant then the indication is that the two food lumps were different, even initially. This analysis is presented in Table 5.5. It can be seen from these results that there are no signifi- cant Chi-square values either in each box or the totals. It may be concluded from this that pairing over time is not due to any dis- crepancies between food lumps. However, once the larvae select a food lump, they begin conditioning it and this cOnditioning may attract the other larva and thus pairing increases. For example, in a 7+l3 situation the 13 day old larva may be doing more conditioning than the 7 and this attracts the 7 to where the 13 is located. We have already seen that whenever a 13 day old larva is present no significant pairing occurs (Tables 5.3 and 5.4) and examination of Figure 5.2 shows the response curve to be random. However, the 7+7, ll+ll, and 7+11 situations show significant pairing with time. In the 7+7 and ll+ll situations we would expect each larva to be doing an equal amount of conditioning and thus excess conditioning by one larva which attracts the other larva is probably not the mechanism. It was also noted (Tables 5.3 and 5.4) that 7+7 day old larvae take longer to exhibit pairing than do the 7+11 or ll+ll situations and 224 .35. u 5.33 35.33 u 3533 p53 Ao.33 u 3333 .mo. - A 33 35.3 n 3333 N N a. N .33 3mAm.o 30505.5 om3o.o eoNo.o noooo.o 3535.5 Nm3m.o NNm Am em mm mm em Ne Nmm3.o moooo.o NO3o.o GO3o.o oooo.o No3o.o N5N3.o 333 .mm. NN mN 3N NN on, N 33553 ANNN.o mooo.o 5500.5 3500.5 moooo.o emoo.o omm3.o 3 5553 33.33 533 5N NN 5N AN NN MN 33 m3 53 33 N3 33 NONN.3 eAeN.c mooo.o NNNN.o memo.o 5033.5 5350.5 3A3o.o AAmo.o 3503.0 mNmN.o mme A5 em 3m 3m A5 35 mm om mm ow Ao3m.o 3350.5 3ooo.o 5303.5 OONo.o 53oo.o mANo.o oooo.o OONo.o A3vo.o 5533.5 N 5553 ANN 5N em em mm 33 on mN NN mN AN \+N. 3AeA.o mem3.o ONoo.o NAA3.o memo.o omo3.o OAmo.o 3330.5 AAmo.o 5055.01 @533.o 3 5553 r 553 53 ON A3 N3 e3 33 33 53 m3 m3 533 53 N3 A3 03 m3 33 N3 N3 33 one xen Aeoo ennunz .m9553 poem anon pew mannem oEnn HHe no>e o=35> onoevmunno Hence osn pee compennnu -ceo onoSGmunnu nmonu pee e353 poem ne venom oanu Aeoo no unwed Honey one one arenm em3< -nsu.3ospn>np:n nnonu pee venom oEHp sumo no @533 poem neoo nem oo>noH me mnnom me nonase “open one azegm .mo33o> onoacm .Houeu one on 93:3 poem nooo nem venom oanu sooo me :enusnnnueeo oap seam ea ponenu ununom onoecmungo 3epeu one can: 3.m onsunm an moon» :eoo nem mnmnnoe< onoadmnnnu nuns moneoe noaownueeu m.m 023,—. 225 Table 5.5 (cont'd.) t1 12 t3 t4 t5 t6 Lum 23 21 24 27fi’ 27: 32 154 7+11 P 0.0400 0.0004 0.0359 0.0155 .0007 0.0117 0.1042 Lum 23 23 28 28 29 :36 167' P 0.0361 0.0005 0.0333 0.0130 .0005 0.0112 0.0947 46 44 52 55 56 68 321 0.0761 0.0009 0.0692 0.0285 .0013 0.0229 0.1989 t1 t2 t3 t4 Lump 20 19 15 15 69 13+13 0.5566 0.3620 0.2200 0.2200 1.3586 L 20 24 25 25 94 “PP 0.4060 0.0157 0.1632 0.1632 0.7481 40 43 40 40 163"' 0.9626 0.3777 0.3832 0.3832 2.1067 t1 t2 t3 t4 Lum 20 19 *23' 21 83 7+13 P 0.0526 0.1097 0.0454 0.0116 0.2229 Lum 18 22 21 22 83 P 0.0526 0.1097 0.0454 0.0116 0.2229 38 41 44 43 166 0.1052 0.2194 0.0908 0.0232 0.4458 t1 t2 t3 t4 Lump 12 18 16 16 62 11+13 0.1365 0.0452 0.0225 0.0005 0.2047 Lum 27 32 29 31 119 P 0.0721 0.0230 0.0113 0.0003 0.1067 39‘ 50 4s 47 181 0.2086 0.0682 0.0338 0.0008 0.3114 *x2 - 3 84 x2 = 7 81 and x2 = 11 07 at p = 05 (1) ° ’ (3) ° ’ (5) ° ' ° 226 that the 7+ll group exhibits the greatest amount of pairing. Since initial preference in relation to food lump is random, we would not expect Table 5.5 to shed any light on why pairing is occurring over time, even if the larvae are changing those food lumps and making them more "attractive" to the other member of the pair. However, we can approach the question of whether something one larva did to one food lump is attracting the other larva in another way. Let us hypothesize that the older larvae do more conditioning than the younger larvae over time and that the younger larvae are attracted to that conditioning. We can test this hypothesis using the 7+ll, ll+l3, and 7+l3 situations. In these groups every dish in which the older larva initially selected one of the plain food lumps and remained there for the whole experiment was selected for analysis. There were 60 such dishes in the 7+ll group, 68 in the 7+l3 group, and 65 in the ll+l3 group. Those food lumps are now designated as the conditioned lump and we can ask how often the younger larva is attracted to that lump, either because of the presence of the other larva or the conditioning. The results of this analysis are in Table 5.6 and graphed in Figure 5.3. The results demonstrate that the 7 day old larva is homing in on the lump with the 11 day old larva in it, but that neither a 7 or 11 day old larva are attracted to the lump with the 13 day old larva in it. It will also be noted that the response vectors in Figure 5.3 are very similar to those in Figure 5.2 B in which pairs over time were initially graphed. 227 Table 5.6 Results of Hotelling's One-Sample analysis on the preference of the younger larva of a pair for the food lump in which the older larva initially selected and remained in for the entire experimental period. Comparison is made against an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. Group Fl 54P(5)11+13 2.10 52P(5)7+13 2.26 5.P(5)7+ll 10.27" ' = = = : F1(4,65) 3.00, F1(4,68) 2.99, and F1(4,6l) 2.63 at P .025. H = :- F1(6,6l) 4.37 at P .001. Of the 12 dishes in group 52P(S)7+l3 in which the 13 did not initially select and remain in a food lump, in 5 dishes the 7 moved away when the 13 moved to its lump and in 3 dishes the 13 moved away when the 7 moved to its lump. In group 54P(5)1l+13, of the 19 dishes in which the 11 did not stay in its initial choice, the 7 moved away 3 times when the 11 moved to its lump and the 11 moved away 4 times when a 7 moved to it. In group 54P(5)ll+13 of the 15 dishes in which the 13 did not choose and stay in a lump, in 5 dishes the 11 moved away when the 13 moved to its lump and in 4 dishes the 13 moved away when the 11 moved to its lump. From the results of Experiment I it is possible to develop the following hypothesis. Only pairs 7+7, 11+11, and 7+ll are sig- nificant over time and whenever a 13 day old larva is one of the test larvae, pairing does not occur, regardless of the age of the other larva. In the cases where significant grouping is found it is 1001 90- 80J 70‘ 60~ sol 404 30‘ % Younger Larvae of Each Pair in "Cond." Food 228 3.... I,” v—-—-** 51P(S)7+11 X—fl SZP (5)7+l3 e... ..... 54P(5)11+13 V r U I I wt r V T V j T V 3 4 5 6 8 9 10 ll 12 13 14 Time in Days Figure 5.3 Mean response vectors of the younger larvae in a pair to a particular "plain" feed lump that was initially selected by the older member of the pair and in which the older larva remained in for the entire experimental period. That food lump is considered the "conditioned" lump for purposes of these curves. 229 not because of some difference between the two food lumps, rather it appears to be due to the presence of another larva or to something that larva does to the food. When it was possible to distinguish individuals, it was found that 7 day old larvae would move to where an 11 day old larva was and stay there. There are two possibilities for this occurrence: either the younger larva prefers to be with the other larva, or the other larva, being older and bigger, is more highly conditioning its food lump than is the 7 day old larva and this attracts the 7. However, there would also appear to be an animal- animal interaction in this behavior since neither a 7 or 11 is so attracted to where a 13 day old larva's lump is the the 13 should be doing the most conditioning. It is possible that the 13 day old larva is doing a different kind of conditioning that younger larvae do not find as attractive. However, the response of 7 and 11 day old isolates, Chapter IV, to similar degrees of conditioning are very strong, indicating there is not a qualitative conditioning difference. In situations where both larvae are the same age, 7+7, ll+1l, and l3+l3 it is not possible to definitively determine what is going on. The initial 7+7 and ll+ll situations exhibit significant grouping with time, the l3+l3 does not. It would appear that a 13 day old larva will not tolerate the presence of any other larva, but younger larvae will. However, both 7 day old larvae should be equally con- ditioning their environments as should both 11 day old larvae. One possible explanation for grouping in these situations is on the basis of animal-animal interactions not conditioning. That is, the presence of older larvae overrides the preference for conditioning by younger 230 larvae, whereas the presence of a younger larva does not. We will come back to these points in the discussion section. To be sure that any one food lump was sufficient for develop- ment of two larvae of any age and that the distributions observed in my experiments are not due to food limitation, the following was done. One hundred and twenty dishes were set up with one food lump in each dish. In 60 of these dishes the lump was highly conditioned (the maximum conditioning of any experiment in this paper) and in 60 dishes it was plain. Two 7 day old larvae, two 11 day old larvae, and two 13 day old larvae were raised in 20 dishes in each situation and their pupation time measured. If the developmental time of these larvae is not different among the groups then food was not a limiting factor in any of the experiments presented in this paper. The results in Table 5.7 show that development was not different from group to group. Table 5.7 Analysis of Variance on developmental times of two 7, two 11, and two 13 day old larvae reared on single conditioned or plain food lumps. Source of Variation d.f. S.S. M.S. F ratio Between Group 5 3.5416 0.7083 0.9723 Within Group 114 83.05 0.7285 Total 119 86.5916 F(S,119) = 2.2914 at P = .05. 231 Experiment 11: When Initial Condition- ipgfis Present, is Preference for Con- ditioning Affected by the Presence of a Conspecific, the Age of the Con- specific, and Whether the Con- specific is a Resident in the Conditioned Food? These experiments examine whether the preference of a larva for conditioning is affected by the presence of a conspecific when that conspecific is an established resident in the conditioned lump or when it is introduced with the other larva. The experimental designs for these experiments are shown in Figures 5.4 and 5.5. Each experimental group has a sample size of 80 and the identification numbers were explained earlier. Only two ages of test larvae are used, 7+l3 days old, and only two degrees of conditioning, high and low. Isolate larvae, of all ages, are attracted to both degrees of conditioning (see Chapter IV), but at a lower level to the low conditioning than to the high conditioning. These two degrees of conditioning, namely LOW: l,12,(7,l3) and HIGH=7,6,(7,13) were chosen because the conditioning larva is 13 days old at the end of the conditioning period in both groups and may then be left in the conditioned food as a resident larva for some of the experiments. To maximize the probability of finding animal-animal interactions only 7 and 13 day old larvae are utilized throughout the remainder of these studies. The experimental procedure for Figure 5.4 is as for all previous experiments. When the conditioning period is over, for each degree of conditioning, the conditioning larvae are removed and a pair of test larvae added, the pairs being either 7+7, 7+l3, or 13+13. In 232 LOW CONDITIONING HIGH CONDITIONING Age of Test Larva Age of Test Larva 7 13 7 13 56PL(1-3)7+7 57PL(1-5)7+13 63PH(1-3)7+7 64PH(l-S)7+13 o E 7 .4 2/5/75 2/5/75 2/5/75 2/5/75 ‘2 Q) n-._ 44 ° 58PL(l-3)13+l3 65PH(1-3)13+l3 3° '13 2/5/75 2/5/75 591L(1)7 6OIL(1)13 66IH(1)7 67IH(1)13 2/5/75 2/5/75 2/5/75 2/5/75 Figure 5.4 Experimental design (top) for animal-animal interactions, at two levels of conditioning, when the conditioning larva is removed and both test larvae are new to the experimental situation. LOW COND. HIGH COND. ,Age of Test Larva 7 13 2/5/75 61PL(1-S)7fl§. 2/5/75 68PH(1-5)7+l§ 13 Age of Test Larva 2/5/75 62PL(1-3)13f§§ 2/5/75 69PH(1-3)13+l§_ larva. J Figure 5.5 EXperimental design (left) for the effect of a resi- dent larva on the prefer- ence of a test larva for biological conditioning. The resident larva is the original conditioning 233 Figure 5.5, however, at the end of the conditioning period the con- ditioning larva is left in the conditioned lump and a single 7 or 13 day old larva added. Both experiments utilize two test larvae, but in one, both are new to the situation and in the other, one is a resident (always a 13 day old larva) and the other is new to the situation (either a 7 or 13 day old larva). Table 5.8 lists each group from Figures 5.4 and 5.5 which have been analyzed, their mean response (preference), their estimated variance (82), and the D-values for serial correlation. It must be remembered that there are several ways in which to analyze any one experimental group based on (1) total larvae in the conditioned lump, (2) pairs in the conditioned lump, (3) pairs regardless of lump, (4) only the 13 day old larva in the conditioned lump and (5) only the 7 day old larva in the conditioned lump. The groups are listed as such in Table 5.8. However, not all of the comparisons were made because they were not necessary to answer the questions being asked and some comparisons would be meaningless. To find out if larvae in pairs are attracted to low and high conditioning, each group was compared to an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. This analysis used the total number of larvae on conditioned food to determine if two larvae distribute themselves randomly or are attracted to the conditioned food. The results in Table 5.9 demonstrate that, regardless of degree of conditioning or age combination of larvae, all groups are significantly different from random and are attracted to conditioned food as can be seen by examining their mean response vectors in 234 Table 5.8 This table presents the response mean vectors(X), estimated variance vec- tors(82), and D-values for serial correlation for the groups in Figures 5.3 and 5.4 to be used in future cppparisons. LEW CONDITIONING PTime in Days Groups 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 9 10 D ’x .637 .643 .650 .643 .643 .712 .781 .743:.781 .681 1.46* 56PL(1)7*7 82 .120 .134 .142 .134 .147 .119 .113 .139 .132 .115 x .412 .450 .475 .450 .475 .537 .662 .637 .575 .375 1.46* 56PL(2)7*7 52 .245 .251 .253 .251 .253 .252 .226 .234 .247 .237 i .562 .675 .700 .681 2.20 57PL(1)7*13 82 .123 .115 .099 .109 ssz(1)7+13 x .562 .675 .700 .681 .718 .612 .531 .443 .431 .375 1.60* Wandering 52 .123 .115 .099 .109 .119 .133 .091 .076 .068 .092 x .312 .450 .475 .462 2.20 57PLC2)7*13 52 .218 .251 .253 .252 X .650 .725 .775 .762 2.11 57PLC4)7+13 52 .230 .202 .177 .183 X .475 .625 .625 .600 .687 .750 .775 .750 .750 .625 1.40* 57PL(5)7*13 82 .253 .237 .237 .243 .218 .190 .177 .190 .190 .237 x .837 .856 .856 .825 2.39 58PL(1)13+13 52 .075 .071 .058 .077 x .737 .750 .725 .687 2.25 58PL(2)13+13 s2 .196 .190 .202 .218 591L(1)7 x .537 .550 .660 .662 .737 .725 .750 .725 .687 .525 1.39* 52 .252 .251 .226 .226 .196 .202 .190 .202 .218 .253 1 750 775 787 837 2 31 601L 1 13 - - - - - C 3 s2 .190 .177 .169 .138 5 IL 1 7 + _ 631LE1;13/2 x .643 .662 .725 .750 .781 .706 .562 .437 .418 .337 x .843 806 787 798 2.24 PL 7+13 : - - 61 (1) -—- 52 .054 .060 .062 .061 X .687 .612 .575 .587 2.24 61PL(Z)7+1§- 2 .218 .240 .247 .245 x .700 .637 .600 .625 .675 .775 .812 .850 .800 .612 1.43* 4. 61PL(5)7 52- $2 .213 .234 .243 .237 .222 .177 .154 .129 .162 .240 x .862 .893 .900 .875 1.961 62le“HYPE-s .063 .049 .047 .073 x 750 812 812 800 2 061 L 2 13+13 - - - - - 62p ( ) -—-s2 .190 .154 .154 .162 * - .. DL(10,80) . 1.71 and DU(10,80) _ 2.28 at P - .05. 1 . . D-value inconclusive, * = = = DL(4,80) 1.89 and DU(4,80) 2.10 at P .05. 235 Table 5.8 (cont'd.) HIGH CONDITIONING Time in Days Groups 1 2 3 4 s 6 7 8 9 10 0 .600 .693 .743 .743 .762 .750 .762 .768 .787 .768 1.26* 63P”(1)7+7 82 .129 .092 .082 .082 .082 .101 .120 .120 .100 .120 x .375 .450 .525 .525 .562 .575 .687 .700 .662 .650 1.30* 63P"(2)7*7 52 .237 .251 .253 .253 .249 .247 .218 .213 .226 .230 x .743 .706 .718 .712 2.031 64PHC1)7*13 52 .108 .099 .100 .119 64PH(1)7+13 X .743 .706 .718 .712 .750 .737 .506 .506 .525 .425 1.27* Wandering S .108 .099 .100 .119 .108 .114 .092 .066 .063 .032 64PH(2)7+13 X .575 .487 .512 .537 1.981 82 .247 .253 .253 .252 x .862 .812 .837 .800 2.24 64PHC437+13 52 .120 .154 .138 .162 x .625 .600 .612 .637 .725 .787 .762 .825 .875 .850 1.27* 64P“(537*13 52 .237 .243 .240 .234 .202 .169 .183 .146 .111 .129 1 .650 .750 .806 .806 2.12 65P“(1)13*1382 .135 .108 .079 .079 x .462 .600 .650 .650 2.071 65P"(2)13*1352 .252 .243 .230 .230 66IH 1 7 x .737 .837 .887 .862 .875 .900 .900 .900 .925 .800 1.49* C 3 52 .196 .138 .101 .120 .111 .091 .091 .091 .070 .162 67mm 22351323232323; ggIHEI313+ X .850 .900 .931 .918 .900 .868 .650 .581 .593 .531 68mm :2 :23 :32; :32; :38 W__ :52 :32: :23: :22: :33; 688527513. £52 :23 :13?) :33 :31? 1333 :13; :‘iii :33? :332 :33} ”5* 69PH9>13+13§2 2333 :31? :313 :323 “7 69P"(2313+.1_3’§g:3§i :33? 1332 :33 “1 :ELCIO'SO): 1.2: ::::U(10’80): :.i: :: : : .::. ID—value inconclusive. L(4,80) ' "(4,30) . . . Hotellings One Sample test on the eXperimental groups in Figures 5.4 Total larvae in the conditioned food (paired, singly, or none) is compared with an expected vector whose entries are all 0. 5. Response mean vectors(X), estimated variance vectors(S2 ), and D-values and S. S. 236 Table 5.9 for serial correlation are found in Table 5. 8. LOW CONDITIONING HIGH CONDITIONING Group F Group F 56PL(1)7+7 6.02* 63PH(1)7+7 9.SO* S7PL(1)7+13 8.23* 64PH(1)7+13 12.98* 58PL(1)13+13 Sl.89* 6SPH(1)13+13 23.83* 591L(1)7 4.56* 661H(1)7 23.75* 60IL(1)13 l8.66* 67PIH(1)13 792.07* 61PL(1)7fL§ 63.53* 68PH(1)7+1§_ 83.70* 62PL(1)13+1§_ $25.50* 69PH(1)13+1§_ 238.89* * F(10,70) a 3.48 and F(4,76) = 5.18 at P = .001. 237 Table 5.8. It must be remembered, however, that when the resident groups are so analyzed, half of the larvae are already in the con- ditioned food. This will be sorted out shortly. It will be noted that the F-values for high conditioning are generally higher than for low conditioning indicating greater attraction to high than low con- ditioning as would have been predicted on the basis of response to conditioning alone. Table 5.10 presents the results of the analysis on whether the number of pairs in conditioned food is significantly different from random. In every case aggregation is occurring in the conditioned food and more so in highly conditioned food. Therefore, not only are total larvae in conditioned food significantly different from random, but of those larvae there are more pairs than would be pre- dicted on the basis of chance alone. Neither of these analyses says anything about whether an individual's preference for the conditioned food has been altered by the presence of a conspecific. That is, are the interactions between the larvae affecting individual behavior? This may be answered by treating each pair of larvae as a unit and comparing it to a similar unit from the isolated situation or by comparing only one member of a pair with an isolate's behavior of the same age. For example, if animal-animal interaction has no effect on an individual's preference for conditioning, then the total number of 13 day old larvae, in the situations where two 13 day old larvae are present, in conditioned food, should not be different from the response vector of isolated 13 day old larvae; or the response vector for the 7+l3 situation should 238 Table 5.10 Hotelling's One Sample test on the experimental groups in Figures 5.4 and 5.5. Pairs of larvae on conditioned food are compared with an eXpected vector whose entries are all 0.25. Response mean vectors(X), estimated variance vectors(Sz), and D-values for serial correlation are found in Table 5.8. Low CONDITIONING HIGH CONDITIONING Group F Group F 56PL(2)7+7 7.04** 63PH(2)7+7 8.74** 57PL(2)7+13 4.28* 64PH(2)7+13 11.74** 58PL(2)13+13 34.16** 65PH(2)13+13 13.77** 61PL(2)7+13 24.23** 68PH(2)7+1§_ 49.44** 629L(2)13+1§_ 49.41** 69PH(2)13+1§_ l45.96** *F(10,70) 2.86 and F(4’76) = 4.11 at P = .005. **F(lo,7o) = 3.48 and F(4’76) = 5.18 at P = .001. 239 not be different from the average response of a 7 day old isolate and a 13 day old isolate; or the response of a 7 day old larvae when with a 13 day old larva should not be different from that of a 7 day old isolate's response. Such comparisons have been made in Table 5.11 and graphed in Figures 5.6 and 5.7. Using Hotelling's Two-Sample approach it can be seen from Table 5.11 that there are no significant differences in response on the part of larvae, when grouped or iso- lated, to low conditioning, although the One-Sample method shows some differences. Examination of Figure 5.6 shows all the response curves for respective comparisons to be about the same. However, in every case the comparisons when high conditioning is used demonstrate that response of individual larvae when with a conspecific, regardless of age, is lower than when a similar aged larva is isolated. This is graphically demonstrated in Figure 5.7. It will also be noted that the greatest effect of animal-animal interaction is when at least one of the members of the pair is 13 days old. Therefore, animal- animal interactions lower individual preferences for conditioned food only when that food is highly conditioned. We will come back to these considerations later. In Table 5.12 are found comparisons examining resident versus non-resident effects on individual preferences for low and high con- ditioning, and the mean vectors are graphed in Figures 5.8 and 5.9. Hotelling's Two-Sample test (F2-values in Table 5.12) shows that in low conditioning, whether one of the larvae is a 13_day old established resident in the conditioned food, or both larvae are new to the situ- ation, it has no effect on individual preferences for conditioning. 240 .nm03m.mv HoHHonom ohm mnouoo> oncommoh :mo2m A3.“ 5 moocownoc, msoocomonouoxz .mcomwnomEoo onaom one on» you xwupoa ooamfino>oo u.>u can napoo> popoomxo u.>m .Hoo. u a pm em.v nfimmfi.evmo 6am .vm.m ufimqfi.ofivmm... 6:6 .mH.m unea.vvfio on“ .wv.m "mon.oHVHm... .Ho. u a on av.m unmmfi.qvma 6:6 .66.N "havfi.ofivmo .. 6:8 .mm.m "ASN.6VHG 6:8 .mm.~ "floa.oHVHm .. .mmo. u a on mm.~ anmfi.¢vmm 6:8 .mH.N unaqfi.ovao . 6:“ .oa.~ "no“.vVHm 6:6..VN.N "moa.oflvfio . : gm.~ ...HH.6H mHfiHUquo .m> mH+mHfiHVgamm a : ON.O .om.~ noHVgHmm .m> mH+anmvganm a : mH.H .am.~ mHflHVOHoo .m> mfl+anevqoam mm.H mH+nfi~Vgaem .m> «\mHAHVquo + amfingom a : w6.H ...mm.m a+nflfivgaom .m> hhHUgHmm amfifiofiflwowa 5869 No Hm .>u .>m pom pooh H m.xom mcomflhomaou N oszoHeHazoo zoo .omo ouofinmoammo o:u mo oo>poH one mo oncommou oau mo oaom on» on pasonm .omo :fi Honcoaa ohm oo>HmH umou ozu opp con: .om>HmH nonuwo How Hoooo> omcommon ocp om~< .omm oeom on» mo o>hmH oco mo omcommon onp mo oamm onp on stosm omm fiasco mo oo>hoH 03H pom muouoo> omcommou ocu .cowpoououcfi Hmawco -Hoe«:o o: mafiESmm< .¢.m oHSMMm a“ museum on» macaw mcomflnmmaoo ofimaom hivoze paw n.voco m.m:wHHouo= HH.m oanuh 241 .fimo.w.mu HoHHonom ono mn0p0o> oncommon coo: a . Go...“ as moononnoc» mascocomonoaflhI .mnomnnomaou ofimamm oco onu nom xnnuoa ooconno>oo u.>u woo nouoo> wouoomxo u.>m a u .noo. u a on 66.6 n Ammn won“ and eH.w u amen envmo... q a .no. u a pm nv.m u nmmn coma 6:8 66.N u amen enema .. a a q .Hoo. u a pm w6.m u non Onvnm... .mmo. u a pm mw.~ n mmmn evmm 6:5 mn.~ u amen onvmo . a : ...mw.m mnnnvmnno .ms mn+mnnnvmamo : ..wo.m nfinvznoo .m> mn+nflmvmaeo a m .m~.m mannumnno .m> mn+nflvvmavo ...mn.6 mH+nflnvzavo .m> «\mnnnvznne + nnnvznoo a : .mm.~ nfinvznoo .m> n+nflnvmamo amfinonnonmd pooh Nu Hm .>u .>m now noon NH m.xom mcomnnomeOu UZHZOHHHQZOU ~1on fl.c.ocooV nn.m ensue 242 Figure 5.6 Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.11) of two larvae situations with one larva situations to find if individual preference for low conditioned food is affected by the presence of a conspecific. A. 591L(1)7 vs. 56PL(1)7+7 B. 6OIL(l)l3 vs. 58PL(1)13+13 C. 57PL(1)7+13 vs. 591L(1)7 + 6OIL(1)13 2 D. 591L(l)7 vs. S7PL(S)7+13 E. 57PL(4)7+13 vs. 6OIL(1)13 243 U whom an oEnH Emmwmpw N 22:86 + 2333 Fill. a 3.23.53 {Illa Lro rom .ov .00 .on SS mxmo an oEnH cromrwmwmmw 'PUOD UT GBAJB'] 96 P003 2313.53 win}... 3 233% [Ill “Ho Tom vow tom rOOH 'puog uI 88A181 % 9005 o.m onswww < whoa an oEnb n o m w m m P D b ' 7:31:18 c.1113. 2333 III. .om fiooH o 6 'puo3 u: eenxeq POOd 244 mxmo an oanh 0H m w u o m v m III I I P I .P P 23.38 gill... 2:93.53 tllll N vom .0? .00 .om .OOH Pooa 'puoo at 984191 910 £90 91 % fl.o.0:663 o.m unsun5 a mxmo an oanb 0H m w n o m w P b b I ’ P P I | 21.33.53 III... 23.5% 9111...“. .0w '0 \O pooa 'puoo HI aeAze1 910 £90 L % .om fiooH 245 Figure 5.7 Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.11) of two larvae situ- ations with one larva situations to find if individual preference for high conditioned food is affected by the presence of a conspecific. A. B. 66IH(1)7 vs. 63PH(1)7+7 67IH(1)13 vs. 65PH(1)13+13 64PH(1)7+13 vs. 66IH(1)7 + 67IH(1)13 2 661H(l)7 vs. 64PH(5)7+13 67IH(1)13 vs. 64PH(4)7+13 n.m unsun5 U m < mxmo an oEnH mxmo an oenb mxmo an oEnn pwun .m h m m m m, m H 02 m m m m m w M 1m 2 omirm m m w m N m m1 w Po Ho F0 N .. . .. 2A32$ + 2328 III: .0... 2.223252 :1... 1.. Ho... $2353 I! 1.. .om 2+235371§ 232$ .11.... 2328 III-1 .05 .05 .os o/0 0/0 "I. n1 B B .om M .0m M .om B B a 9 I. TL. .00 u .00 u .00 3 3 m m... p. x s. .2 . x .0... . .2. m . m D. \\x 0. flow r.....» .8 .8 .05 .om .om .03 .03 .2: venxeq % 'puog u; 9009 247 2.0.00003 5.0 onsmno m n. whom an osnh mxmo an oEnn 02 0 0 n 0 m 6 m N 2 02 0 0 n 0 m 6 m N 2 III! b P D D D P I| P O P b b r r I P L b3 P o H H 2.263.560 x... I I a 4. 2.23560 0...... .. 1x -1 .00 .6 .00 022230250 6:111:11 m“ 5223:200 .1111114 .06 .m .06 K 0 I .00 o. .00 "I H m V00 9 too I. \KIIK u u m. .00 m_ .00 .00 .00 .\\u111\\x .002 .002 poog 'puog up aeAJeq p10 Keg L g 248 .HoHHonmm ono mnopoo> omcommon :0o25 .mmo.mvmv mooconnm> mooooowonopo:25 .mcomnnomsoo oHQEom oco onu nom xnnuoa ooconno>oo u.>o poo nouoo> oouoomxo n.>m Q a a A a A .5N0. u 5 26 00.N "2552 63N5 6:6 .52.N "2062 023N5 . 606 .00.N u 205 6325 606 .6N.N "205 02325 . 5 05.0 .mw+m22N325N0 .m> 02.522N32505 5 5 0N.2 .05.N 02.52532555 .m> 52232205 5 5 20.N ...00.N .mm+52532520 .m> 52232200 5 5 50.2 ...00.0 52.52532555 .m>.MM+N2532520 05226226205 2565 N5 25 .>0 .>5 now noon H m.xom moomnnomsou N UZHZOHHHQZOU 304 .o>n02 omoommon ogp can: mesoonounaanm oooooonucn o>no2 nonuooo mo uoommo onu mo oaom onu on m>n02 omcommon on“ no o>nm2 ucoonmon o mo uoommo oSH Mn mooconoMMno now on no: omHo onsosm ononh .moomnnomEoo omonu on mooconommfio moo on no: oaoonm onosp mconuoonoucn HoannoIHosnco o: mana:mm< .mcncononoooo HoonmoHonn now ooconomonm :o muoommo ucoonmonncoc msmno> unoonmon ocnanouoo on m.m woo 6.m monsmnm on masonm Houcoannomxo onp macaw mcomnnomsoo oamaom 083039 paw n.3oco m2mcn22opo: NH.m oHan 249 .Howwonom ono mnouoo> omcommon coo:n2 . 30. m2: 082262.262, mooocomonouozr .mcomwnoaeoo oHQsom oco ocu now xnnpoe oocownm>oo u.>u woo nouoo> oouoomxo u.>m .200. u 5 26 60.6 "2002 63N5 6:6 .6N.0 "2062 02355... 656 .02.0 6265 6325 666 .06.0 6205 02325... .20. u 5 66 56.0 "2002 63N5 666 .06.N "2062 023N5 .. 606 .00.5 "265 6325 606 .00.N "205 02325 .. .0N0. u 5 66 00.5 «2002 63N5 6:6 .02.N "2062 023N5 . 6:6 .60.N "205 6325 666 .6N.N 6205 02325 . 5 ...05.N2 02+022N3=506 .6>.MM+M22N35506 : .00.0 52235206 .6> 02.52035566 : N0.N ...06.0 52235206 .6>.mm+5203:500 : .06.N 02.52035566 .6>.mm+5203:506 a226226.265 2665 N5 25 .>0 .>5 now pooh me m.xom mcomnnomaou 022202520200 :02: 2.6.60663 N2.5 62065 250 Figure 5.8 Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.12) of resident versus non-resident effects on larval preference for low conditioning. A. 61PL(5)7+13_vs. 57PL(5)7+13 B. 61PL(5)7+13_vs. 591L(l)7 C. 57PL(5)7+13 vs. 59IL(1)7 D. 62PL(2)13+1§_vs. 58PL(2)13+13 100' 90< 80'I 70' 601 % 7 Day Old Larvae in Cond. Food 251 I A -—__.. 61PL(S)7+_1_3_ 30 x— --x 57PL(5)7+13 45 J2 . . - - - - .02. l 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 9 10 Time in Days 100‘ c 901 '0 o o u. 6' a o L) : -H o o E o .—'l 'o H c: >. g 40 h 69 '-—--v 57PL(5)7+13 30 " x---l 591L(l)7 0T v v v I 1 v v u ' v 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Time in Days 10m 90' 40 3 7 Day Old Larvae in Cond. Food 30 l r % Paired in Cond. Food 3( -——-—- 61PL(5)7+_1_3_ 1— --'! 591L(l)7 f v v v v 23356789 Time in Days H: -—-——-—-o 62PL(2)13+1§_ { -- - -¥ 58PL(2)13+13 J Figure 5.8 1 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 9 10 Time in Days 252 Figure 5.9 Mean response vectors for comparisons (Table 5.12) of resident versus non—resident effects on larval preference for high conditioning. A. B. 68PH(5)7+l§_vs. 64PH(5)7+13 68PH(5)7+1§_vs. 661H(1)7 64PH(5)7+13 vs. 66IH(1)7 69PH(2)13+1§_vs. 6SPH(2)13+13 253 100' 0 O A m C A \1 ‘2 Ox ‘2 U1 C2 .1: Ca ~—-————. 68PH(S)7+_1_3 % 7 Day Old Larvae in Cond. Food k» - --x 64PH(5)7+13 30‘ .J. 01:, , . , . . f . ,fi 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Time in Days 100‘(: ’1 ¢*‘*‘f \ 90' f‘f’fi’ \ \1 9. ox Cl 01 Q 4:. Q ‘—-—-o 64PH(S)7+13 % 7 Day Old Larvae in Cond. Food x- - --r 66IH(1)7 u 'LQ °—+ 3 4 S 6 7 8 9 10 Time in Days 1 5 Figure 5.9 100‘ 90 'U 8 . m 80 'u' 8 U 70 I: .fi 3 60‘ E t6 .—1 T, s H 0 >4 8 4 ~———-‘ 689H(5)7+_1_3_ l\ a? 3 *- '"" '1 66IH(1)7 - Y i 5 5 s 6 7 8 53—16 Time in Days 100‘ D 80' '6 O O D- .70 72 2"" '3 « 560i ' I ““350 ’ -H g 7 6"4 o——-———< 69PH(2)13fl§ 3( X“- - -l 65PH(2)13+13 I5 41 E5 (3 77 1% S) l () Time in Days 1 2 254 All larvae behave as if isolated. The One-Sample Fl-values, however, indicate some differences possibly due to the difference in initial preferences as can be seen in Figure 5.8, although these differences are probably due to heterogeneous variances. The comparison 64PH(S)7+13 versus 66IH(1)7 is depicted for comparative purposes, as is comparison 64PH(S)7+13 versus 66IH(1)7. However, there are individual behavioral differences when conditioning is high, Table 5.12, and Figure 5.9. A 7 day old larva's preference for high conditioning is lowered when the 13 day old larva is introduced with it, but is not affected when the 13 day old larva is the resident in the conditioned food lump. The preference curve for the 7 day old larva when the resident is present is the same as the 7 day old isolate curve. This indicates that the significant animal-animal interaction is taking place when the 7 and 13 are initially placed in the center of the dish and not in the food lump. I will come back to this point in the discussion section. The two 13 day old larvae cannot be distinguished so the comparison was based on the number of pairs in conditioned food and it is noted that a 13 day old larva with a 13 day old resident is not affected but it is if both 13 day old larvae are introduced together. In the next experiments I have selected only the situation where the 7 and 13 day old larvae are introduced together into a situation with high conditioning. These experiments will show that there is an interaction between biological conditioning and animal— animal interactions in the responses we have been examining. 255 Experiment III: Are the Lowered Responses of Younger, 7 Day Old Larvae, When a 13 Day Old Larva is Simultaneously Introduced, a Result of the Changing Biological Conditioning, Interaction with the 13 Day Old Larva, or Both? In the last experiment we saw that a 7 day old larva was rela- tively unaffected by a 13 day old larva if the 13 day old larva was a resident in the highly conditioned food, but its preference for conditioned food was significantly lowered if the 13 day old larva was introduced at the same time. However, re-examination of the response vectors (see Table 5.12 and Figure 5.10) shows that the 7 day old response gradually rises to a level where it would have been if the 13 day old larva had not been present. In Figure 5.10, I have also graphed the response vector for the 13 day old larva including its wandering phase at which time it leaves the conditioned food and moves about the dish until a suitable pupation site is located and pupation occurs. Notice that as the 13 day old curve falls the 7 day old curve rises, the two crossing at about day S of the experiment, or when the 13 day old larva is 17 1/2 days old and the 7 day old is 11 1/2 days old. This suggests that when the 13 day old larva enters the wandering phase and vacates the conditioned food lump, the 7 day old larva moves to that lump, or that the 13 day old larva moves over to the 7 day old larva's lump, and in some way inter- acts with it causing the 7 day old larva to shift food lumps. There may, of course, be an interaction between the two. Since it is the older larva that seems to be determining the behavior of the 7 day old larva, older larva, i.e., 13 day old, will be used for manipulative purposes and only the 7 day old response will be monitored. % Larvae in Cond. Food 1001 90' 80J 70‘ 40‘ 30' 20' 10‘ 256 fl \ )I—x—x x- - - ——x 64PH(4)7+13 ‘-'--'0 64PH(S)7+13 I I I I I r T j I v I r v 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 9 10 ll 12 13 14 Time in Days Figure 5.10 Mean response vectors of a 7 day old larva and a 13 day old larva with the wandering phase of the 13 day old larva included. These are from experimental group 64PH(l-5)7+13. 257 If the rise in the 7 day old response curve is a function of some interaction due to the wandering phase of the 13 day old larva, we should be able to shift the 7 day old curve two days earlier (to the left) or two days later (to the right) by pairing 7 day old larvae with 15 or 11 day old larvae, respectively. To test this hypothesis, the following 4-group experiment was designed. In one group, a 7 day old and a 15 day old larva were introduced, in another, a 7 and 13, and in a third a 7 and 11 were used. The fourth group is a 7 day old isolate larva with high conditioning. All experimental procedures are as previously described and one of the two food lumps was highly conditioned. For identification purposes the 4 groups are: 70PH(1-S)7+15 71PH(l-S)7+13 72PH(l-5)7+11 73IH(1)7 The mean response vectors (i), estimated variance vectors (82), and D-values for each 7 and the ll, 13, and 15 day old larvae from these 4 groups are presented in Table 5.13 and graphed in Figure 5.11. However, only comparisons among the 7 day old response curves are of interest, but the response of the older larvae are shown for compara- tive purposes. Table 5.14 shows the results of the One- and Two-Sample comparisons among the three 7 day old preference curves when in the presence of high conditioning and either a 15, 13, or 11 day old con- specific. It can be seen from the results of this experiment that the response to high conditioning of a 7 day old larva when paired with 2 Table 5.13 58 This table presents the response mean vectors(X), estimated variance vectors(S2 ), and D- values for serial correlation for the groups to be analyzed in Table 5.14. HIGH CONDITIONING Time in Days Group 1 2 3 4 s 6 7 8 9 10 D x .587 .612 .725 .837 .912 .887 .900 .900 .925 .837 1.41* 7°?"(5)7*15 $2 .245 .240 .202 .138 .081 .101 .091 .091 .070 .138 x .600 .625 .587 .625 .725 .712 .825 .850 .875 .850 1.38* 71PH(5)7+13 82 .243 .237 .245 .237 .202 .202 .146 .129 .111 .129 X .625 .587 .600 .612 .675 .700 .687 .837 .900 .812 1.55* 72P“(5)7+11 $2 .237 .245 .243 .246 .222 .213 .218 .138 .091 .154 73IH(1)7 i .712 .887 .875 .887 .925 .900 .900 .912 .900 .850 1.62* 82 .207 .101 .111 .101 .070 .091 .091 .081 .091 .129 2 .850 .900 .912 .812 .800 .425 1.831 7OPH(4)7*15 32 .129 .091 .081 .154 .162 .247 x .850 .837 .850 .850 .787 .612 .337 .087 1.39* 71PH(4)7+13 32 .129 .138 .129 .129 .169 .240 .226 .081 X .837 .825 .862 .887 .837 .837 .875 .700 .250 .050 1.55* 72PHC437+11 $2 .138 .146 .120 .101 .138 .138 .111 .213 .190 .048 * DL(6,80) — 1.83 and DU(6,8O) = 2.16 at = .05. * DL(8,80) = 1.77 and DU(8,8O) 2 2.22 at = .05. * DL(10,80)= 1.71 and DU(10,80)= 2.28 at = .05. I . . D-value was inconclu51ve. 259 Figure 5.11 Mean response vectors of 7 day old larvae to high conditioning when a 15, 13, or 11 day old conspecific is also present. The mean response vectors for the 15, 13, and 11 day old larvae are shown, with their wandering phases, for comparative purposes. Illustrated are the mean response vectors for: A. B. 7 and 15 day old larvae. 7 and 13 day old larvae. 7 and 11 day old larvae. All the 7 day old response vectors from A, B, and C, as well as the response vector for a 7 day old isolate to the same initial degree of conditioning. 260 100' 90‘ on 0 LL \1 Ci 60q l--“*~-~ % Larvae in Cond. Food 0'- -' -- -¢ 70PH(4)7+15 ' '_'-—-‘ 70PH(5)7+15 J" j 153586789. Time in Days 100 90 00 q \I Q Larvae in Cond. Food o. ’o A 9 w- - -- w 72PHC4J7+11 3EL —— 72PH(S)7+11 0T V U ‘ 123415678910 Time in Days .Figure 5.11 1001 B 901 80‘ -u 0 0 LL. , 70 'o a 8 :60‘ -H ‘ 8 > 5 \ ’3 1 .—l o\° ‘ 4 x l .........71PH(4)7+13 \ \ -—--—~71PH(S)7+13 ‘L‘m‘ ' i E 3 i 2'; 6 7 8 876 Time in Days lOO‘lE) ,“'-’¢O" 90‘ ’ I 801 [I l l \I Q if". Larvae in’Cond. Food —SL- 'v ’/ 1 ”)4 so °“°4 .—-————. 7OPH(5)7+15 x 1: 71PH(S)7+13 3 o... N.-. 72PH(5)7+11 -----~ 73IH(1)7 12345678910 Time in Days 261 Table 5.14 Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons of the 7 day old mean response vectors when paired with an ll, 13, or 15 day old larva or when an isolate, all with initial high conditioning on one food lump. Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism 71PH(5)7+13 vs. 70PH(5)7+15 2.24* H 71PH(5)7+13 vs. 72PH(S)7+11 2.16 0.90 H P 72PH(5)7+11 vs. 70PH(5)7+15 2.87* H 73IH(1)7 vs. 70PH(S)7+15 7.08"' 1.96 H 73IH(1)7 vs. 71PH(S)7+13 2.60* H 73IH(1)7 vs. 72PH(5)7+11 3.19* H ' Fl(10,70)= 2.24 and F2(10,149)= 2.15 at P = .05. " 81(10,70)=2.59 and ** F2(10,149)= 3.59 at p - .01. "'Fl(10’70)=3.48 and ***F2(10,149)= 5.18 at P = .001. EV.= expected vector and CV.= covariance matrix for the One Sample comparisons. HHeterogeneous variances (P PMean response vectors are parallel (P‘&. E .05). 05). 262 a 15 day old larva is significantly higher than its response when paired with a 13 or an 11 day old larva. However, the 7 day old response when paired with a 13 day old larva is not different from when paired with an 11 day old larva. In Figure 5.11, it can be seen that as either the 15 or 13 day old larvae enter the wandering phase, their response curves begin to fall and that of the 7 day old response to conditioning begins to increase. In the 7+ll situation, however, the 7 day old response is on the rise before the 11 day old larva enters the wandering phase. It can be seen also that the response of a 7 day old larva to high conditioning is significantly lower when an 11 or 13 day old larva is present than when the 7 day old larva is alone. The 7 day old response when with a 15 day old larva, however, is not different from the isolated condition, although it would be at the .05 level, and is different with the One-Sample test, indicating a problem due to heterogeneous variances. All comparisons, except one, yield non-parallel responses indicating the possibility that the 7 day old larvae are utilizing different behaviors in their response when a conspecific is present, than when isolated. These responses also appear to be different with different aged conspecifics, except in the 7+ll versus 7+l3 situation. It is very tempting to say that the response of’7 day old larvae to conditioning is lower because it has been repelled by the older larva. However, in Table 5.15 are presented the results of comparing the first three days of each 7 day old response mean vector to a vector whose entries are all 0.5 to see if repulsion has occurred. These results show (in combination with Figure 5.11) that the initial 263 Table 5.15 Hotelling's One Sample (') comparison of the first 3 days of the 7 day old response vectors when with various aged conspecifics and when isolated, with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. Comparisons Fl 7OPH(5)7+15 7.12" 71PH(5)7+13 1.74 72PH(5)7+11 1.95 73IH(1)7 43.15" I = = F(3’77) 3.29 at P .025. u = = F(3’77) 6.02 at P .001. responses, although lower than if isolated, are all either not dif- ferent from random or significantly higher than random. Therefore, although the presence of an older conspecific significantly lowers the response of a 7 day old larva to conditioning, the term repulsion is not appropriate since the responses are not significantly lower than random. These results are suggestive that the response of a 7 day old larva to conditioning when paired with an older conspecific may be the result of an interaction between biological conditioning and animal- animal interaction. The initial lower response in each case being due to some interaction between the 7 day old larva and the conspecific in the center of the dish before either larva has sampled the food lumps. The rise in the 7 day old response with time, however, is more difficult to explain. Except when paired with an 11 day old con— specific, the 7 day old response seems to coincide with the 13 or 15 264 day old wandering phase. It may be that when the older larva leaves the conditioned lump it goes over to the 7 day old's lump and a second interaction occurs causing the 7 day old larva to move to a new food lump. However, it must be remembered that when the older larva chooses the originally conditioned lump, it spends at least a day, and in the case of the 13 day old larva, 3 days, adding its own conditioning to that already present. Therefore, when the older larva leaves this lump, the 7 day old larva may be responding to the extra conditioning. There may also be an interaction between these two events. The following experiment was designed to look at these possibilities, using only the 7+13 situation since the greateSt effects on the 7 have been with a 13 day old larva. The next hypothesis to be examined is that the response of the 7 day old larva to conditioning when a 13 day old larva is present is due to an interaction between three variables: (1) the initial encounter between the two larvae in the middle of the dishes, (2) the extra conditioning that the 13 day old larva adds to the already con- ditioned lump, and (3) a second interaction between the two larvae when the 13 begins wandering. These events will henceforth be referred to as l, C, or 2 respectively. The following experimental groups were set up with various combinations of the above variables: 74IH(l)7(O-O-O) : This is the 7 isolate situation having none of the above variables operating. 7SPH(S)7+13(1-C-2): This is the situation in which a 7 and 13 day old larva are placed in the experimental situation and the 7 is monitored for the total experimental period. 265 76PH(5)7+13(l-0-0): In this group the 7 and 13 day old larvae are placed in the experimental situation and in 12 hours the 13 is removed. There- fore, the 7 received the initial encounter but not the second encounter and there was no extra conditioning being done by the 13, or at least only a very small amount in 12 hours. 77PH(5)7+13(l-C-0): The 7 and 13 were placed in the apparatus and when the 13 day old larva was 16 days old, just before wandering, it was removed from the dish. Thus, the 7 received the initial encounter and the extra conditioning, but not the second encounter with the 13 day old larva. 78PH(5)7+13(l-O-2): The 7 and 13 were placed in the dishes and 12 hours later the 13 was removed. Four days later, or when the 13 would have begun wandering, a wandering larva from the colony was placed in the dishes. There- fore, the 7 day old larva received the initial encounter and second encounter but no extra conditioning had been done by the 13 day old larva. Each of these 5 groups received different manipulations through- out the experimental period, and as a control for this the following was done. Whenever the 13 day old larva was removed from the dishes in the respective groups, the dishes in the groups not receiving this treatment were opened and the food lumps gently tapped with a pair of forceps. When a wandering larva was added to group 78PH(5)7+13(l—O-2) all other dishes were opened and the center floor of the dish gently tapped with forceps. The mean vectors (X), estimated variance vectors (82) and D-values for serial correlation are shown in Table 5.16 for the 7 day old test larvae in each group. The results of Hotelling's One- and Two-Sample comparisons between all possible combinations of 7 day response vectors from Table 5.16 are shown in Table 5.17. The 266 wom.owva wom.0wvmo a. .mo. u m an wm.mn Q cam H5.H u mew. omw. emw. emw. emw. wmw. oew. mew. mew. mew. wm .eo.w wmm. ome. wmm. wmm. wme. mwm. wHe. wmm. wmm. wmm. w fio-o-wvmw+wwmvma6w mww. wow. 6mm. mww. wow. www. oew. mew. mew. mew. wm .mm.w oow. mww. wmw. oow. www. wmm. wwm. com. wmm. wmm. w wo-u-wvmw+wwmvzaww wow. mmw. mew. wow. mew. mew. oew. oew. wew. wmw. wm lwm.w mww. wow. wmw. com. www. wmw. wwm. wwm. mwm. mwm. m Aw-o-wvmw+wwmvzwww wmo. HHH. mew. mmw. omw. wow. wmw. mew. mew. mew. wm .mw.w mwm. mwm. mwm. wow. omw. www. mwm. wmm. com. com. m ww-u-wvmw+wflmvzamw mwm. owo. owo. Hmo. How. owe. Hem. oww. mmm. wow. wm .mm.w omm. mwm. mwm. com. wmm. mwm. wmm. wmm. wmm. mww. x wo-o-ovwfiwvmwew nammwmmemwm mango mwhmH vac mew w on» we comuawonhoo Hawnom pom monam>ua use .flmmeHOpoo> oocmwum> woumawumo .Amvmnouoo> omcommou cams mo manmb eH.w ofinmh 267 Table 5.17 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(*) Sample comparisons among the 7 day old response vectors in Table 5.16. Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism (l-C-Z) vs. (0—0-0) 2.17* H P (1-0-2) vs (0-0-0) 2.93** H P (l-C-O) vs. (0-0-0) 3.28** H P (1-0-0) vs (0-0-0) 3.73*** H P (1-0-2) vs (l-C-Z) 2.31' 0.85 H P (l-C-O) vs. (l-C-2) 2.19 0.95 H P (1-0-0) vs (1-C-2) 4.35"' 1.58 H P (1-C-O) vs (1-0-2) 0.97 0.44 H P (1-0-0) vs. (1-0-2) 2.11 0.99 H P (1-0-0) vs (l-C-O) 0.68 0.35 H P O * F1(10,70) - 2.24 and F2(10,149) = 2.15 at P = .025. II ** F1(10,7O) I 2.59 and F2(10,149) = 2.46 at P = .01. It! *** F1(10,70) - 3.48 and F2(10’149) = 3.24 at P = .001. EV.= expected vector and CV.= covariance matrix for the One Sample Test. HHeterogeneous variances (Pf .05). 1)Mean response vectors are parallel (P:’: .05). 268 response mean vectors for the 7 day old larvae from all groups are graphed in Figure 5.12. (For the purposes of identifying these groups in the tables and figures, I have used only the three numbers for the variables in question, i.e., l-C-2, l-C-O, 1-0-0, 1-0-2, or O-O-O. Since all groups deal with only the response of the 7 day old larva this system makes it clearer which variables are being referred to.) It can be seen from the Two-Sample comparisons (Table 5.17) that the 7 day old isolate response to conditioning is significantly higher than all the others, but that there are no other significant differ- ences. The One-Sample test elucidates only two other differences. All responses are parallel, however, indicating similar behaviors but at different levels. These results were a bit surprising in light of the separation of response curves in Figure 5.12. However, examination of the data indicates some possible reasons for the results obtained. The range in which the responses have in which to separate themselves out is a relatively narrow one during the time in which the responses begin to exhibit differences; from about 60% to 85%. The variances are also heterogeneous, as attested to by Box's test and the results of the One-Sample test. Throughout this chapter, as well as Chapter IV, I have alluded to this fact. If we examine the variance vectors for these groups, and in fact for all groups in this Chapter, an interesting trend will be noted. As response to conditioning increases the variances decrease. Therefore, the variance vectors may be a good mirror for larval activity. For example, examine the variance vectors for groups o 6 Larvae in Cond. Food 269 1001 904 80* 70‘ 604 50* 404 30' O-~~ C-I-a-a-O (O'O'O) :- fi (l-C-Z) 20' o . (l-O-O) I 1 (1-0-2) 10‘ ‘-------o (l-C-O) 0 V V f 1 t I 1 I j v v v r fl 1 3 4 S 6 7 8 9 10 11 12. 13 14 Time in Days Figure 5.12 Response mean vectors for the 7 day old larvae from all experimental groups in Tables 5.16 and 5.17. 270 l-C-Z and 1-0-0 and notice that in Figure 5.2, as the response vector for l-C-2 rises, the variances get smaller, perhaps indicating a more stable activity of the larva as they select and stay in the conditioned food. The variances for group l-O-O, however, remain large, and as can be seen, the response vector for this group never rises much. That is, this group does not get to conditioned food and their activity is high or fluctuating. This is the most probable explanation for the lack of significant results in the comparisons in Table 5.17. Because of the heterogeneous variances, it was decided to analyze the last three data points for each of the response vectors. It can be seen from Table 5.16 and Figure 5.12 that the variances have stabilized, although still large in some groups, and the response vectors have relatively stabilized at this time. We would not expect to get some of the same differences in this comparison since some of the responses are now at the same level. For example, groups l-C-2 and 0-0-0 are no longer different because l-C-2 is at the same level as 0—0—0 in the latter stages of the experiment. Table 5.18 shows the results of these comparisons. The only new difference now detected is that l-O-O is a lower response than l-C-2. Tables 5.17 and 5.18, therefore, indicate the following. All manipulations produced significantly lower responses on the part of the 7 day old larvae than for the response 0f isolated 7 day old larvae. That is, the presence of an older larva, even if only for 12 hours, is a sufficient stimulus to prevent the response of 7 day old larvae to conditioning from reaching the level of an isolate's response. This is also a sufficient stimulus for preventing 7 day 271 Table 5.18 Results of Hotelling's One(') and Two(") Sample comparisons among the 7 day old response vectors in Table 5.16 for the last 3 days of each mean response vector. Comparisons Box's T2 Test for EV. CV. F1 F2 Test Parallelism (l-C-2) vs. (0-0-0) 4.96" 1.58 H P (1-0-2) vs. (0-0-0) 10.ll"' 2.81 H (l-C-O) vs. (0-0-0) 3.95* H P (1-0-0) vs. (0-0-0) 7.66*** H P (1-0-2) vs. (l—C-Z) 5.69" 2.01 H P (l-C-O) vs. (l-C-2) 6.87"' 2.77 H P (1-0-0) vs. (l-C-Z) 4.40** H P (1-0-0) vs. (l-C-O) 0.94 P (1-0-2) vs. (1-0-0) 3.84' 1.67 H P (l-C-O) vs. (1-0-2) 0.20 P ' 32 d * Fl(3’77) - . 9 an F2(3,162) = 3.22 at P = .025. t! *1: Fl(3,77) = 4.05 and F2(3,l62) = 3.94 at P = .01. 'Il *** Fl(3,77) - 6.01 and F2(3,162) - 5.78 at P = .001. EV.= expected vector and CV.= covariance matrix for the One Sample Test. HHeterogeneous variances (P $.05). pMean vectors are parallel (p $.05) . 272 old larvae from reaching the same response level as a 7 day old larva that received the two encounters with the older larva and the extra conditioning. The other two groups are intermediate. That is, the groups that received both encounters but no extra conditioning or only the first encounter with extra conditioning are statistically not different from the group that received only the first encounter or the group receiving all three stimuli. However, the trend for the response vectors of these two groups is that they lie between the 1-0-0 and l-C-2 groups. It would also appear that the l-C-O group is closer to the l-C-Z group. This may indicate that the extra con- ditioning is not as necessary to overcome the initial encounter (i.e., l-C-O) as is the second encounter without the conditioning (i.e., 1-0-2). The F2-values also indicate this trend. Although suggestive, these "trends" are not significantly different from one another. It would also appear that these experiments have not yet made it possible to make definitive statements about certain aspects of the interaction between biological conditioning and animal-animal interactions, although there are some strong indications. This will be discussed in the discussion section. A point made earlier in this chapter is worth repeating at this time. Even though the 7 day old response vectors are signifi- cantly lowered by the presence of an older conspecific, they are not being repulsed. If repulsion were the mechanism, then we would expect the response vectors to be significantly below random, i.e., below 509. Table 5.19 shows the results of comparing the first 5 days of response for each experimental group, since it is during the 273 Table 5.19 Results of Hotelling's One Sample comparison of the first 5 data points of each group from Table 5.16 with an expected vector whose entries are all 0.5. Group F1 0-0-0 277.62"' l-C-2 4.22" 1-0-2 13l.42"' l-C-O 2.78' 1-0-0 1.31 ' = = F(5’75) 2.74 at P .025. H = = F(5,75) 3.28 at P .01. m = = F(5,7S) 4.64 at P .001. first half of the experimental period that all responses are lowest. The results show that all groups are either random or significantly above random. These differences would not be as great if fewer data points had been used, but the point is that all groups, except group l-O-O, are significantly different from random in the direction of attraction to conditioned food and therefore not repulsed in the strict sense of the word. Group l-O-O is also not repulsed but is randomly responding. This may indicate the need for a second stimulus to counteract the effect of the initial encounter with the 13 day old larva. Throughout this chapter, several groups have been replicated for various experiments. In particular, the 7 day old isolate group 274 and the 7 day old group when paired with a 13 day old larva have each been replicated three times. Table 5.20 presents the results of com- paring these replicates to see if they differ from one experiment to the next. Table 5.20 demonstrates that all comparisons are not signifi- cantly different with the Two-Sample test, but that one is different with the One-Sample test. This is the first hint that populations of larvae may be genetically changing with time. Discussion Invertebrate studies indicate that spatial distribution is a function of an interaction between biological conditioning and animal- animal interactions. These studies, however, are population studies in which it is impossible to separate individual behavior from group behavior, and there has been no systematic attempt to do so. In Park's work (1934, 1935) the distribution of Tribolium confusum was concluded to be heavily dependent on the amount of homotypic con- ditioning. However, as population size increases, not only does con- ditioning increase, but the number of adults and larvae increases. What appear to be responses to increased conditioning may well be responses to increased density or an interaction between the two. Naylor (1959) attempted to separate these variables and was partially successful. His results indicate that spatial patterns in adult Tribolium are primarily dependent upon interactions between adults as density increases but that these patterns are modifiable when sexual relationships and biological conditioning are introduced as variables. However, Naylor was not able to determine the relationship between 275 . Goiwn: 3:33 one muouoo> omcommou cmozd . amo. w my moocomuo> msoocowowouoxz .pmoe owmaom oco on» pom xwwuos ooaomuo>oo u.>o one wouoo> ooooomxo u.>m q q .wo. . a we me.w u Amew cwvww.. use mm.w u how cwvwm.. .mwo. u a we mw.w u nmew vawm . ea“ ew.w u how oHVHm . a m Hm.o ew.w mw+wfimvzaem .m> mw+whmvzwmw a : wm.o ew.w mw+wfimvmaww .m> fiw-u-wvmw+wfimvzamw a x wm.o mm.o mw+wnmvzwem .m> Aw-u-wvmm+wflmv:wmw a z wm.o Hw.w wo-o-onw+wnszHew .m> wflwvzmmm a : om.o ww.o wo-o-onw+wwwV:Hew .m> wmmvmmmw a m we 8 .mm w wflssmwem ..> wwscmwmw smwwomwewew umow wm Hm .>o .>m Ham pooh Na m.xom mnemmnomaou .uoumono was» unocwsonnu powwowamou mmsoum maowno> mo mconwuomaoo onsom awvozh one m.vo:o m.m:«HHouo: mo mHHSmom ew.w ofinmh 276 biological conditioning and adult interactions partly because he did not utilize a graded series of conditionings. Wellington's (1957) work with tent caterpillars was free of the adult-young interactions of Park's (1935) work, but he did not determine whether the preferences of these larvae for homotypic conditioning was affected by the presence of conspecifics. In 1963a and l964e, Surtees concluded from his studies on grain weevils that spatial patterns in high densities of weevils are a result of changes in individual behavior as a result of stimulation from conspecifics. However, his studies are confounded by the presence of high biological conditioning which he neither con- sidered nor controlled for. A previous paper (Chapter IV) demonstrated that the distribution of isolate larvae is a function of larval conditioning and that pref- erence for conditioned medium is a function of the degree of condi- tioning and the age of the responding larva. The rationale for my studies in this chapter has been that the preference of individual Galleria larvae for homotypic conditioning is a function of the degree of conditioning, the presence of a conspecific, and the age and loca- tion of the conspecific. Individual Responses to a Conspecific and Biological Conditioning. When there is no previously conditioned food available, larvae tend to distribute themselves randomly when isolated or when a con- specific is present. There is, initially, neither an attraction or a repulsion between pairs of larvae regardless of age. With time, however, larvae begin to aggregate, apparently as a result of their 277 own conditioning and possibly due to the development of interactions between different aged larvae. It was demonstrated, for example, that if one member of a pair of larvae is a 13 day old larva, aggregation does not occur and larval distribution remains random. Younger larvae do begin to aggregate and the most significant case was the 7 and 11 day old situation. Analysis further demonstrated that the 7 day old larva is moving to the food lump on which the 11 day old larva is residing, probably as a result of the 11 day old larval conditioning being greater. However, such aggregation behavior would also neces- sitate that each larva tolerate the close proximity of another larva, whereas this is not the case with a 13 day old organism. The lack of aggregation with 13 day old larvae is probably not because 13 day old larvae are secreting a qualitatively different conditioning than younger larvae, since isolate 7 or 11 day old larvae have been shown to be attracted to the same degree of conditioning when the 13 day old larva is absent (see Chapter IV). Situations with two 7 or two 11 day old larvae also exhibit aggregation behavior. This is probably not a case of one larva's conditioning being greater than the other and thus attracting the other larva, although there may be individual differences in con- ditioning behavior. Younger larvae do not initially respond to each other, but when they each begin conditioning and connecting the food lumps with silk tunnels this may increase the probability of finding them in pairs. This behavior may also be related to activity rhythms. Long (1955) demonstrated that different ages of various butterfly larvae exhibit different activity rhythms. He found that the closer 278 to pupation these larvae get, the more "restless" they become. This restlessness does not necessarily mean they move about more, only that they are more easily agitated and exhibit rather sudden and jerky movements when touched. Younger larvae tend to be more docile and move about with more easy-going motions. Therefore, young larvae may tolerate the presence of young, or not very old larvae, since their activity rhythms are more synchronous whereas the activity patterns of older larvae are disruptive to group formation. Experiment II indicates that larval interactions are associ- ated with the presence of an already conditioned food lump. The pref- erence behavior of individual larvae to low-conditioned food is the same whether isolated or with a conspecific, regardless of the age of the conspecific. As seen in Chapter IV, however, the response of isolate larvae to this degree of conditioning, particularly for a 7 day old larva, is already at a relatively low level and it would almost require repulsive behavior to detect a difference between the isolate and group situation. When one food lump is highly conditioned, however, the preference of individual larvae is affected by the pres- ence of a conspecific, regardless of the age of the conspecific. For example, the total number of 7 day old larvae on conditioned food when two 7 day old larvae are present is lower than when only one larva is present. This distinction is difficult to elucidate since the two larvae are not individually distinguishable. However, it can be definitively seen that a 13 day old larva's response to conditioning is lowered when a 7 day old larva is present and a 7 day old's response is lowered when a 13 is present, as compared to the isolate 279 situation. This suggests that although the greatest effect is on the 7 day old larva's response, the 7 day old larva also affects the response of an older larva. The response of individual larvae to a conditioned food lump of low or high conditioning is unaffected by the presence of a resident larva (13 day old larva) in that lump. That is, individuals respond to such a situation as if the resident were not present. The conditioning may act as a buffer, allowing organisms to be paired that do not pair in the absence of conditioning. It is also possible that the test larva and the resident never encounter one another in the conditioned lump. Experiment III elucidated the results of Experiment 11, in that 7 day old response to highly conditioned food is lowered when a 15, 13, or 11 day old larva is introduced with the 7. There may be a relationship between the wandering phase of the older larva and the rise in response of the younger larvae. The 7 day old response begins increasing two days earlier when a 15 day old larva is present than when a 13 day old larva is present, the wandering phase of a 15 day old larva occurring two days sooner than for a 13 day old larva. However, the response vector of the 7 day old larva begins rising earlier than predicted when an 11 day old larva is present. This may be a function of the initial encounter with an 11 day old larva not being as traumatic for a 7 day old larva as it is with a 13 or 15 day old larva. This result also supports the finding in Experiment I that 7 day old larvae are attracted to where the 11 day old larva is residing. 280 Further experiments attempted to dissect the behavior of the younger larva with the hypothesis that its lower response was due to an interaction between the initial encounter with the older larva, the extra conditioning the older larva adds to the already conditioned food lump, and a second encounter when the older larva begins wander- ing. The results demonstrated that any combination of these variables is sufficient to significantly lower the younger larva's response from that of the isolate situation. It was also found that the situation in which only the initial encounter with the older larva was the variable that response was significantly lower than when all three variables were present. No other differences were found, although there is a decided trend for the situations in which both encounters are present or only the first encounter and extra conditioning were present, to be intermediate between the others. There are several reasons why these trends were not significant and they will be dis- cussed in the next section. The general conclusion from all of these studies is that indi- vidual behavior and distribution are functions of the degree of homo- typic conditioning, age of individual larvae, presence of a conspecific, and location of the conspecific. There are some indications from these studies that differences in larval activity may play a role in spatial distribution. By larval activity I am referring to the time in development when larvae tend to move from one food lump to another, but not movement within any one food lump. For example, it was demon- strated in Chapter IV that 7 day old larvae are less active during the first 2—3 days of the experiment, as measured by the number of times 281 they move from one food lump to another, whereas 13 day old larvae are more active at that time. Mobility differences, on the other hand, refer to the ability of larvae to move from one food lump to another. It was previously pointed out (Chapter IV) that older larvae may be more mobile than younger larvae and can more easily cross the open space between food lumps. In the next section I will discuss the possibility that dif- ferences in larval activity may play a role in the spatial distribution of Galleria larvae, and propose several hypotheses for future testing. The discussion, however, will be mostly theoretical. Possible Mechanisms for the Interaction of Biological Conditionigg and Larval- Larval Interaction in the Spatial Distribution of Galleria Larvae In all of the experiments presented in this chapter, the only situations in which larval response to conditioning was significantly lower than in the isolate situation were when larvae initially encountered each other in the center of the experimental dishes and when a highly conditioned food lump was present at the start of the experiment. Assuming high conditioning to be a preferred stimulus, some sort of competition may be involved in the larval interactions for it. However, we saw in Chapter IV, that older isolate larvae exhibit a higher initial preference for highly conditioned food than do younger larvae, and it was postulated that this is related to activity differences between the larvae. Older larvae find the con- ditioned food lump sooner than young larvae and cease their movements in the preferred stimulus, although differential sensory capabilities 282 may also be involved. The young larvae, on the other hand, tend to remain in the first food lump encountered for a period of time and move to the conditioned food lump later in development. If now, a younger larva encounters an older larva in the center of the dish and if the older larva is preferentially heading for the conditioned food, the younger larva may merely head in the opposite direction as an "escape" reaction, which would enhance the normally lower initial response of young larvae. I have also observed that these larvae release some sort of liquid substance if disturbed. Upon encountering each other, in the center of the dish this substance may be released, causing avoidance between the two larvae and differentially having a greater effect on the younger larva. It is not clear what consti- tutes the interaction, but it is clear from these experiments that the initial interaction is a prerequisite for lowered responses to conditioned food. It is also indicated that this interaction is less traumatic when between two larvae of not very different ages. For example, the 7 day old larva's response curve rises relatively sooner when the interaction was between a 7 and 11 day old larva than between a 7 and 13 or 15 day old larva. When the 7 day old response curve begins to exhibit a rise, 1 have postulated that it is a result of a second interaction between the two larvae, or a response to the extra conditioning the older larva has done to the already conditioned food, or an interaction between the two. These variables may be linked to activity differences. After the initial encounter between the two larvae, the 7 day old response to conditioning is depressed and remains so for about four 283 days. At the end of those four days the 13 day old larva is entering the wandering phase and leaves the conditioned lump. At the same time, the 7 day old larva, which is now 11 days old, may be more active and begins to explore its environment. This was indicated in Chapter IV. It thus encounters the highly conditioned food which is more highly conditioned than the food it just left and it remains there. Thus its response to conditioning increases. This would explain why the 7 day old response increases sooner when an 11 day old larva is present but which is not yet wandering. If this were the only mechanism, however, we would predict that the response curve for the group in which only an initial encounter with the 13 day old larva took place, to begin an upswing about four days into the experiment and reach the same level as the situation in which both encounters and extra conditioning were present. Experiment III, however, demon- strated that those two situations remain significantly different. Of course, in the situation where only the initial encounter occurred, the highly conditioned food lump did not receive any extra condition- ing. However, if the 7 day old larva does enter a more active phase, it should have found that lump and remained there since it was still more highly conditioned than its own lump. Some other mechanism(s) must be operating. It was noted earlier that the variances for the response of larvae to conditioning, whether paired or isolates, decrease as response to conditioning increases. This may indicate that less move- ment between food lumps occurs as larvae find and stay in the con- ditioned food. This is particularly obvious for the situation where 284 a 7 day old isolate has a choice between a plain and a conditioned food lump. As more and more larvae find the conditioned lump, they remain there and activity, as measured by exchanging food lumps, ceases. In the situation where a 13 day old larva was introduced with the 7 day old larva, we find that about 60% of the 7 day old larvae initially choose the conditioned and 40% the plain lump. However, perhaps there is movement between the two lumps. That is, there may be a constant exchange between the two food lumps. This would not be the case if the 7 had been isolated for the whole experiment and the hypothesis is that the initial encounter with the 13 day old larva may have in some way altered the activity of the 7 day old larva and that a second stimulus, i.e., extra conditioning, a second encounter with the 13, or both is needed to drive the 7 day old response up to the level where it would have been if it had been an isolate to begin with. Long's (1955) data tends to support this postu- lation. He demonstrated that young larvae are relatively less active than older larvae of the large white butterfly and that older larvae will disrupt the activity rhythm of a young feeding group. Based on the foregoing considerations the following system is being postulated for future testing: 1. Young larvae are initially less active, as measured by exchanging food lumps, than older larvae but their activity increases as they age. This was demonstrated in Chapter IV and is also evident from my data sheets with young isolate larvae in a situation with two initially plain food lumps. The response in such a situation is always random over time, 285 but about 5 days into the experiment, many of these isolates switch food lumps and remain there. 2. High degrees of conditioning are a preferred stimulus and when encountered activity decreases. This was also earlier demonstrated and is supported by examining the variances for such situations. The variances decrease as highly conditioned food is encountered. 3. Older larvae, being more active, have two effects on younger larvae. They depress the younger larva's response to highly conditioned food and possibly alter its activity in such a manner that young larvae tend to remain in the initially chosen food lump. 4. Young larvae, which have been so affected by an older larva, may require a second stimulus to offset their tendency to remain in the initially chosen non-conditioned food lump and to find and settle in the conditioned food when the older larva is gone. Such a second stimulus may be in the nature of a second encounter with the older larva or the presence of extra conditioning. There may be an inherent activity rhythm in these larvae, in which, as they age, they become investigatory and begin sampling their environment and moving between food lumps. If a highly conditioned food lump is encountered they may cease this activity and settle down. If they had been in such a conditioned food lump initially they may sample the plain lump but return to the preferred stimulus. If young larvae initially encounter an older larva in the open, i.e., not in a 286 conditioned lump, the effect is to alter the young larva's behavior so that its preference for conditioned food is lowered and its activity is affected in such a way that normally conditioned food is not a sufficient stimulus to offset it. Therefore, many exchanges might occur between plain and conditioned lumps. If, however, the larva receives a second stimulus in the form of a second encounter with the older larva or extra conditioning, or both, its activity may be offset and it settles in the preferred highly conditioned lump. This hypothesis is partly supported by my data, but much of it is only circumstantially supported and several tests are needed, all of which relate to an individual's behavior and activity. The fact that activity rhythms do differ among different ages of larvae is tentatively supported by the data in Chapter IV. However, high conditioning is a preferred stimulus and even active larvae will settle in it, although they may continue to sample their surroundings. The experiments in this paper support the idea that older larvae alter the behavior of younger larvae and lower the young larva's response to conditioning. However, the hypothesis that a second stimulus, in the form of extra conditioning or a second interaction with the older larva, is needed to increase the younger larva's response to conditioning is not well supported. An increased sample size may be needed to pull these relationships apart. One of the major problems is that we cannot be sure that a second encounter with the older larva actually occurs, and if it does, whether it has any effect. This is a function of the experi- mental design. The first encounter occurs in the center of the dish 287 when both larvae are introduced into the apparatus. If the second encounter occurs, however, it would have to occur in the food lump in which the younger larva is residing, and cause it to leave that food lump and move to the highly conditioned one. Yet in Experiment 11 the data indicates that a larva's preference for conditioned food in which an older larva is residing is unaffected by that older larva. The situation in which an older larva moves to conditioned food con- taining a young resident was not tested and we do not know what the effect on either larva would be. This needs to be done. We also do not know if any interaction occurs between two larvae in a conditioned food lump. Such a situation may buffer any interactions demonstrated to occur in the open (in the middle of the dish) and it is in fact, possible for two larvae not to encounter each other, at least directly, in a food lump. Tests are needed in which the experimental situation has been so altered as to maximize the chances of two larvae meeting in the food lumps. One possibility is to decrease the size of the food lumps. A better approach might be to so construct the conditioned food lump that only enough food is available for the survival of one larva and ask the question which larva is ejected. The other part of the hypothesis that is not well supported is that extra conditioning may be a required stimulus to offset the lowered activity of the younger larva after an initial encounter with the older larva. This may be easily tested by the following compari- son. In the final experiment of this paper group (l-O-O) received an initial encounter with the older larva but no extra conditioning or 288 second encounter. It was found that the 7 day old response curve does not significantly rise. That is, even when the older larva is removed the younger larva does not move to the conditioned food. This group should be rerun, but after about 4 days an even more highly conditioned lump would be added to the situation. If extra conditioning is a factor in offsetting the increased activity, the younger larvae should move to this new lump and remain there. Finally we need more information about the general activity of these larvae. For example, the initial encounter described is between a 7 and a 13 day old larva, but the second encounter, if it is occurring, is between a 16 or 17 day old larva (the original 13 day old larva) and an 11 or 12 day old larva (the original 7 day old larva). The two situations may be totally different as to the effects on each larva. The effect of the initial encounter and the second encounter may be a function of the size difference between test larvae. This may be tested by systematically decreasing the age differences between test larvae. The variability encountered in these experiments also needs to be investigated. Variability may be environmental, genetic, or both. Two things need to be done. First, is to begin manipulating the larva's environment to see what happens to the variability. For example, there were no controls in my experiments for the fact that some larvae were reared in more dense colony groups than were others or that older larvae in the colony are subjected to higher degrees of conditioning than were younger larvae. These two factors may have an effect on the activity levels and preference for varying degrees 289 of conditioning. If all larvae were reared as isolates before testing, this may result in different levels of variability. A second approach is to artificially select for a "standard" population of Galleria so that the genetics of my experimental animal is under control. As an extension of this I would also like to select for a population of larvae that do not spin silk and begin asking questions about whether the behavioral response to conditioning is genetically related to the physical act of spinning. Once these variables are under control, the next step would be to make predictions about the behavior of larger larval populations, and experimentally test the predictions. CHAPTER VI SUMMARY The studies in this dissertation deal with the preference behavior of individual Galleria mellonella larvae for homotypically conditioned food, its relationship to spatial distribution, and the effects of degree of conditioning, developmental age, and presence of conspecifics on these preferences. A mechanism is postulated for the interaction of these variables with larval activity in determining spatial patterns. Also presented is an analytical technique for the analysis of multiple samples taken from the same individuals over time. Isolate Behavior 1. Response of isolate larvae to homotypic conditioning is either random or attraction, depending on the degree of conditioning used. 2. Within a particular age of test larvae, response to condition- ing increases as the degree of conditioning increases up to a certain level; the degree of conditioning being measured in terms of the number of days conditioning occurs, the age of the conditioning larva, or both. A threshold effect is 290 291 indicated, in which degrees of conditioning above a certain level do not elicit a greater response. There appears to be an age difference in response to con- ditioning. Older larvae exhibit a greater response than younger larvae, at least initially, to the same degree of conditioning. However, these age differences cannot be separated from early experience differences of the larvae with colony density and colony conditioning, or from length of experience with the experimental situation. Age differences in initial response to low versus high degrees of conditioning are more pronounced between young and old larvae. Older larvae exhibit higher discrimination between degrees of conditioning than do younger larvae. A difference in activity, as measured by larval movement between food lumps, between young and older larvae was indicated. Both exhibit increased movement to conditioned food as conditioning increases, but older larvae do most such movement in the earlier periods of the experiment and young larvae move more 3 or 4 days later. Activity differences are supported by the variance vectors for each group. As response to conditioning increases, the variances decrease, whereas the variances remain large when larval response to low conditioning is low. This indicates that more movement between food lumps occurs when one of the lumps is non-conditioned and the other has a low degree of conditioning. As conditioning increases, larvae select the 10. 292 conditioned food and tend not to continue moving between food lumps. Larval movements and preference for conditioning are affected by food source. Larvae exhibit an initial preference for deficient—conditioned food but move to normal non-conditioned food within 24 hours. The initial response to deficient- conditioned food is also initially lower than to normal- conditioned food. This may be a function of the conditioning itself or the food source. Female (adult) Galleria avoid highly, larval-conditioned food for egg laying purposes and this was interpreted in terms of an ability to interpret high conditioning as high larval population and a behavior of laying eggs in situations where larval density is low. Interactions Between Conspecifics and Biological Conditioning_ Pairs of larvae initially distribute themselves randomly when placed in a situation where no previous conditioning has occurred. With time, however, significant aggregation occurs between 7+7, 7+ll, or ll+ll day old larvae. Whenever a 13 day old larva is present, significant pairing does not occur. Aggregation behavior is not due to some discrepancy in food lumps but is due to either an interaction between larvae, biological conditioning, or both. For example, pairing in the 7+ll day old situation is due to the 7 day old larva moving to the more highly conditioned food lump where the 11 day old 11. 12. 13. 14. 293 larva is residing. Similar movements do not occur to where a 13 day old larva is residing, probably a function of larval- larval intolerance, not conditioning. When one of the food lumps is highly conditioned, aggregation behavior occurs in that lump, regardless of the age of the two test larvae. However, the response of individual larvae to the conditioning is affected by the presence of a conspecific. If a conspecific is a resident in the conditioned food, the behavior of test larvae is the same as if the conspecific were not present. For example, a 7 day old larva's response to a conditioned food lump with a resident 13 day old larva in it is the same as a 7 day old's response to a conditioned food lump lacking a resident. This behavior is the same whether the food was low or high in conditioning. When both test larvae are introduced into the experimental situation at the same time, their responses to conditioning are lowered if conditioning is high and unaffected if the conditioning is low, as measured against the response of isolates to similar degrees of conditioning. When response is affected, the response of the younger larva is more drastically lowered than is that of the older larva, but eventually increases with time. The younger larva's response was manipulated by varying the age of the older conspecific and it was postulated that the behavior of the younger larva is a result of three variables: (1) the initial encounter with the older larva, (2) the extra 15. l6. 17. 294 conditioning being done by the older larva, and (3) a second encounter with the older larva. Experiments utilizing various combinations of these variables demonstrated that the initial encounter was the factor involved in the lowered initial preference for conditioning, but the effects of extra conditioning and a second interaction were inconclusive. An hypothesis was advanced relating the inter- action between biological conditioning, larval-larval inter- actions, and larval activity rhythms to the observed spatial patterns of Galleria larvae and various tests proposed. A larger sample size may be needed to elucidate these variables Such a sample size may be calculated by N = 3/G(1-G), as dis- cussed in Chapter III. The general conclusion from all of these studies is that indi— vidual behavior and distribution is a function of degree of conditioning, age of the individual, presence and age of a conspecific, location of the conspecific, and possibly the activity of Galleria larvae. Hotelling's One- and Two-Sample T2 Statistic for Multivariate Analyses was utilized for the analysis of the data, with modifications for handling longitudinal data on individuals. This proved to be a very effective way of handling data on individual animals and for comparing the total response of organisms over time. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Adolph, E. F. 1931. The size of the body and the size of the environ- ment in the growth of tadpoles. Biol. Bull. 61:350-375. Alexander, Nancy J. 1970. A regulatory mechanism of ecdysone release in Galleria mellonella. J. Insect Physiol. 16: 277-289. Alexander, P. and D. H. R. Barton. 1943. The excretion of ethyl- quinone by the flour beetle. Biochem. Jour. 37(4):463-465. Allee, W. C. 1926. Studies in animal aggregations: causes and effects of bunching in land isopods. J. Exp. Zool. 45: 255-277. Allee, W. C. 1927. Animal aggregations. Quart. Review Biol. 2: 367-398. Allee, W. C. and J. F. Schuett. 1927. Studies in animal aggregations: the relation between mass of animals and resistance to colloidal silver. Biol. Bull. 53:301-317. Allee, W. C. 1931. Animal Aggregations: A Study in General Sociology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Allee, W. C. and J. R. Fowler. 1932. Studies in animal aggregations: further studies on oxygen consumption and autotomy of the brittle star, Ophioderma. J. Exp. Zool. 64:1-19. Allee, W. C. 1934. Recent studies in mass physiology. Biol. Rev. 9:1-48. Allee, W. C., E. S. Bowen, J. C. Welty, and R. Oesting. 1934. The effect of homotypic conditioning of water on the growth of fishes, and chemical studies of the factors involved. J. Exp. Zool. 68:183-213. Allee, W. C., A. J. Finkel, and W. H. Hoskins. 1940. The growth of goldfish in homotypically conditioned water: a population study in mass physiology. J. Exp. 2001. 84:417—443. 295 296 Allee, W. C., Alfred E. Emerson, Orlando Park, and Thomas Park. 1949. Principles of Animal Ecology, Philadelphia and London: W. B. Saunders Company. Andrews, J. E. 1921. Some experiments with the larva of the bee moth, Galleria mellonella L. Trans. Wis. Acad. Sci., Arts and Letters 20:255-261. Asher, James H. 1970. Parthenogenesis and genetic variability. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Michigan. Ann Arbor, Michigan. Balazas, A. 1958. Nutritional and nervous factors in the adaptation of Galleria mellonella to artificial diet. Acta. Biol. 9:47-69. Beck, Stanley D. 1960. Growth and development of the greater wax moth, Galleria mellonella (L.). (Lepidoptera: Galleriidae). Trans. Wis. Acad. Sci., Arts and Letters 49:137-148. Bell, J. 1940. The heat production and oxygen consumption of Galleria mellonella at different constant temperatures. Physiol. Zool. 13573-81. Borchert, A. 1933. Zur Biologie de grossen Wachsmotte (Galleria mellonella). I. Uber morphologie und Entwicklungsdauer der larvae der grossen Wachsmotte. Zool. Jahr. Abteil. Anat. u. Ont. 57:105-115. Bossert, William H. and Edward O. Wilson. 1963. The analysis of olfactory communication among animals. J. Theoret. Biol. 5:443-469. Bovbjerg, Richard. 1952. Ecological aspects of dispersal of the snail Campeloma decisum. Ecology 33:169-176. Bowen, E. S. 1921. The role of the sense organs in aggregations of Ameiurus melas. Ecol. Mono. 1:1-35. Box, G. E. P. 1950. Problems in the analysis of growth and wear curves. Biometrics 6:362-389. Brand, R. A. 1955. Abundance and activity of the woodmouse (Peromyscus leucopus novebogacensis) in relation to the characteristics of its habitat. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Michigan. Braun. 1867. The bee moth. Amer. Bee Jour. 66:380-400. Breukelman, John. 1932. Speed of toxic action of mercuric chloride on daphnids. Physiol. 2001. 5:207—218. 297 Brindley, H. H. 1910. Further notes on the procession of Cnethocampa pinivora. Proc. Camb. Phil. Soc. 15:576-587. Brown, L. A. and A. M. Banta. 1935. Control of sex in Cladocera. Physiol. Zool. 8:138-155. Brown, M. D. 1946. The growth of brown trout (Salmo trutta Linn.). I. Factors influencing the growth of trout fry. J. Exp. Biol. 22:118-144. Brown, Margaret E. 1946. The growth of brown trout (Salmo trutta Linn.). I. 1. Factors influencing the growth of trout fry. J. Exp. Biol. 22:118-129. Brown, Margaret E. 1951. The growth of brown trout (Salmo trutta Linn.). IV. The effect of food and temperature on the survival and growth of fry. J. Exp. Biol. 28:473-491. Brown, R. Z. 1953. Social behavior, reproduction, and population changes in the house mouse (Mus musculus L.). Ecol. Monogr. 23:217-240. Burkett, B. N. 1962. Temperature block of spinning and deve10pment in Galleria mellonella (Lepidoptera). Amer. 2001. 2:396. Butler, C. G., D. J. C. Fletcher, and Doreen Watler. 1969. Nest- entrance marking with phermones by the honeybee, Apis mellifera L., and by a wasp, Vespula vulgaris L. Anim. Behav. 17:142-147. Calhoun, J. B. 1948. Mortality and movement of brown rats (Rattus norvegicus) in artificially super-saturated populations. Jour. Wildl. Mgt. 12:167-172. Calhoun, J. B. 1950. The study of wild animals under controlled conditions. Annals New York Acad. Sci. 51:1113-1122. Calhoun, J. B. 1952. The social aspects of population dynamics. Jour. Mamm. 33:138-159. Calhoun, J. B. and W. L. Webb. 1953. Induced emigrations among small mammals. Science 117:358-360. Carthy, J. D. 1951. The return of ants to their nest. Trans. 9th Int. Congr. Entom. 1:365-369. Chapman, R. N. 1928. The quantitative analysis of environmental factors. Ecology 9:111-122. Chase, Ruth W. 1921. The length of life of the larva of the wax moth, Galleria mellonella L., in its different stadia. Trans. Wis. Acad. Sci., Arts and Letters 20:263-267. 298 Christian, J. J. 1950. The adren-pituitary system and population cycles in mammals. J. Mamm. 31:247-259. Christian, J. J. 1970. Social subordination, population density, and mammalian evolution. Science 168:84-90. Chiang, H. C. and A. C. Hodson. 1950. An analytical study of popu- lation growth in Drosophila melanogaster. Ecol. Monogr. 20:173-206. Chitty, D. 1955. Adverse effects of population density upon the viability of later generations. In the numbers of man and animals. Edited by J. B. Cragg and N. W. Pirce. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd, Ltd., 57-67. Chitty, D. 1960. Population processes in the vole and their rele- vance to general theory. Can. J. Zool. 38:99-113. Cochran, W. G. 1947. Some consequences when the assumptions for the analysis of variance are not satisfied. Biometrics 3:22-38. Cole, H. A. and E. W. Knight-Jones. 1949. The settling behavior of larvae of the European flat oyster, Ostrea eduli L., and its influence on methods of cultivation and spat collection. Fishery Invest. London, ser 2. l7(3):59 pp. Cole, L. C. 1946. A study of the dryptozoa of an Illinois' woodland. Ecol. Monogr. 16:49-86. Collias, Nicholas E. 1964. The evolution of nests and nest building in birds. Amer. 2001. 4:175-190. Crombe, A. C. 1942. The effect of crowding upon the oviposition of grain-infesting insects. J. Exper. Biol. 19:311-340. Dadd, R. H. 1964. A study of carbohydrate and lipid nutrition in the wax moth, Galleria mellonella (L.), using partially syn- thetic diets. J. Insect Physiol. 10:161-178. Dadd, R. H. 1966. Beeswax in the nutrition of the wax moth, Galleria mellonella (L.). J. Insect Physiol. 12:1479-1492. Davis, D. E. 1958. The role of density in aggressive behavior in house mice. Anim. Behav. 6:207-211. Davis, D. E. and J. J. Christian. 1956. Changes in Norway rat popu- lations induced by introduction of rats. J. Wildl. Mgt. 20: 378-383. Dawson, P. S. 1964. An interesting behavioral phenomenon in Tribolium confusum. Tribolium Inform. Bull. 7:50-52. 299 Dickman, A. 1933. Studies on the wax moth Galleria mellonella with particular reference to the digestion of wax by the larvae. J. Cell and Comp. Physiol. 3:223-246. Doull, K M. 1953. Phase coloration in lepidopterous larvae. London. Nature 172:813-814. Dryl, S. 1963. Contributions to mechanism of chemotactic response in Paramecium caudatum. Anim. Behav. 11:393-396. Drzewina, A. and G. Bohn. 1921. Variations de la susceptibilite aux agents mocifs avec le nombre des animaux traites. C. R. Acad. Sci. 172:485-487. Drzewina, A. and G. Bohn. 1926. Activation par la lumiere des effets de l'argent sur convoluta. C. R. Acad. Sci. 183:677-679. Durbin, J. and G. 8. Watson. 1950. Testing for serial correlation in least squares regression. I. Biometrika 37:409-428. Durbin, J. and G. S. Watson. 1951. Testing for serial correlation in least squares regression. II. Biometrika 38:159-178. Dutky, S. R., J. V. Thompson, and George E. Cantwell. 1962. A technique for mass rearing the greater wax moth. Proc. Ent. Soc. Wash. 64:56-58. Edmondson, W. T. 1945. Ecological studies of sessile rotatoria. II. Dynamics of populations and social structures. Ecol. Monogr. 15:143-172. Edney, E. B. 1954. Woodlice and the land habitat. Biol. Rev. 29: 185-219. Edwards, J. S. 1966. Neural control of metamorphosis in Galleria mellonella (Lepidoptera). J. Insect Physiol. 12:1423-1433. Edwards, T. G. 1910. On the procession and pupation of the larva of Cnethocampa pinivora. Proc. Camb. Phil. Soc. 15:431-436. Ellis, P. E. 1953. Social aggregation and gregarious behaviors in hoppers of Locusta migratoria migratorivides. (R. and F.). Behavior 5:225-260. El-Sawaf, S. K. 1950. The life history of the greater wax moth (Galleria mellonella L.) in Egypt, with special reference to the morphology of the mature larva. Bull. Soc. Fouad. Ent. 34:247-297. Faure, J. C. 1932. The phases of locusts in South Africa. Bull. Ent. Res. 23:293-428. 300 Fitch, John H. 1975. Behavioral habitat selection as a factor in the distribution of woodland deermice, Peromyscus maniculatus gracilis, in the Kingston Plains of Northern Michigan. Ph.D. Dissertation. Michigan State University. Fraenkel, G. S. and D. L. Gunn. 1961. The Orientation of Animals. New York: Dover Publications. Free, J. B. 1961. The stimuli releasing the stinging response of honeybees. Anim. Behav. 9(3-4):193-l96. Friedlander, C. P. 1964. Thigmokinesis in woodlice. Anim. Behav. 12:164-174. . Gee, J. M. 1965. Chemicals stimulation of settlement in larvae of Spirorbis rupestris (Serpulidae). Anim. Behav. 13(1):181-186. Ghent, Arthur W. 1963. Studies of behavior of the Tribolium flour beetles. I. Contrasting responses of I: castaneum and I, confusum to fresh and conditioned flour. Ecology 44(2): 269-283. Ghent, Arthur W. 1966. Studies of behavior of the Tribolium flour beetles. II. Distributions in depth of I, castaneum and I, confusum in fractionable shell vials. Ec010gy 47(3):355-367. Gill, J. L. and H. D. Hafs. 1971. Analysis of repeated measurements of animals. J. Anim. Sci. 33:331-336. Graham, W. M. 1958. Temperature preference determination using Tribolium. Anim. Behav. 6:231-237. Gunn, D. L. and D. P. Prelou. 1940. The humidity behavior of the mealworm beetle, Tenebrio molitor. III. The mechanism of the reaction. J. Exp. Biol. 17:307-316. Harris, V. T. 1956. An experimental study of habitat selection by prairie and forest races of the deer mouse. Peromyscus maniculatus. Contrib. Lab. Vert. Biol. Univ. of Michigan 56:53 pp. Harshbarger, John C. and B. Shain Moore. 1966. Some effects of x- irradiation on larvae of Galleria mellonella. J. Invert. Path. 8(2):277-279. Haydak, Mykola H. 1936. Is wax a necessary constituent of the diet of wax moth larvae? Ann. Ent. Soc. Amer. 29:581-588. Haydak, Mykola H. 1940. The length of development of the greater wax moth. Science 91:525. 301 Heatwole, Harold. 1962. Environmental factors influencing local distribution and activity of the salamander, Plethodon cinereus. Ecology 43:460-472. Hotelling, Harold. 1931. The generalization of student's ratio. Ann. of Math. Stat. 2:360-378. Howard, Eliot. 1920. Territory in Bird Life. New York: Atheneum. Imms, A. D. 1948. A General Textbook of Entomology. New York: Dutton. Issakowitsch, A. 1905. Geschlechtsbestimmende ursachen bei den daphniden. Biol. Centralbl. 25:529-536. Ito, Koichi and William Schull. 1964. On the robustness of the T 2 test in multivariate analysis of variance when variance- covariance matrices are not equal. Biometrika 51:71-82. Jacobson, Martin, Meyer Schwarz, and Rolland M. Water. 1970. Gypsy moth sex attractants: A reinvestigation. J. Econ. Ent. 63(3): 943-945. Jespersen, P. 1924. The frequency of birds over the high Atlantic Ocean. London. Nature 114:281-283. Johnson, R. P. 1973. Scent marking in mammals. Anim. Behav. 21: 521-535. Johnston, James W., David G. Moulton, and Amos Tuck. 1970. Communi- cation by Chemical Signals, Vol. I. New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts. Karlson, P. and A. Butenandt. 1959. Phermones (ectohormones) in insects. Ann. Rev. Entom. 4:39-58. Kaston, B. J. 1964. The evolution of spider webs. Amer. 2001. 4:191-207. Knight-Jones, E. W. and J. P. Stevenson. 1951. Gregariousness during settlement in the barnacle, Elminuis modestus Darwin. J. Mar. Biol. Ass. U. K. 29:281-297. Krebs, C. J. 1966. Demographic changes in fluctuating populations of Microtus californicus. Ecol. Monogr. 36:239-273. Krebs, Charles J., Barry L. Keller, and Robert H. Tamarin. 1969. Microtus population biology: Demographic changes in fluctuating populations of M, ochrogaster and M, pennsylvanicus in Southern Indiana. Ecology 50(40):587-607. 302 Lack, David. 1954. The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Legay, J. M. and J. L. Chasse. 1964. Etude experimental de la sortie d'une enciente chez Tribolium confusum. Acad. Sci. (Paris), Compt. rend. 258:717-719. Lerner, I. M. and N. Inouze. 1968. Behavior genetics with special reference to maze—running of Tribolium. In Dronamraju K. R. (ed.). Haldane and Modern Biology. Johns Hopkins Press. Loconti, Joseph D. and Louis M. Roth. 1953. Composition of the odorous secretion of Tribolium castaneum. Ent. Soc. of Amer., Annals 46:281-289. Loomis, W. F. and H. M. Lenhoff. 1956. Growth and sexual differ- entiation of hydra in mass culture. J. Exp. 2001. 132:555-568. Long, D. B. 1953. Effects of population density on larvae of Lepidoptera. Trans. R. Ent. Soc. Lond. 104:544-585. Long, D. B. 1955. Observations on sub-social behavior in two species of Lepidoptera larvae, Pieris brassicae L. and Plusia gamma L. Trans. R. Ent. Soc. Lond. 106:421-437. Loschiavo, S. R. 1952. A study of some food preferences of Tribolium confusum Duv. Cereal Chemistry 29:91-107. Lwengood, W. F. 1937. An experimental analysis of certain factors affecting growth of goldfishes in homotypically conditioned water. Copeia. 81-88. Magnuson, John J. 1962. An analysis of aggressive behavior, growth, and competition for food and space in Medaka (Oryzias latipes) (Pisce: Cryprinodontidae). Canadian J. of 2001. 40:313-363. Maschwitz, Ulrich W. 1964. Alarm substances and alarm behavior in social hymenoptera. Nature 204:324-327. McDonald, D. J. and L. Fitting. 1965. Activity differences in black and wild type strains of Tribolium confusum. Tribolium Inform. Bull. 8:124-125. McDonald, Daniel J. 1968. Mobility in Tribolium confusum. Ecology 49:770-771. Milum, V. G. 1952a. Characters and habits of moth larvae infesting honeybee combs. Amer. Bee Jour. 92:200-220. Milum, V. G. 1952b. Descriptions and habits of the adults of some moths whose larvae infest combs of honeybees. Amer. Bee Jour. 92:155-156. 303 Milum, B. G. and H. W. Geuther. 1935. Observations on the biology of the greater wax moth, Galleria mellonella L. J. Econ. Ent. 28:576-578. Morrison, Donald F. 1967. Multivariate Statistical Methods. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Myers, Judith and Charles J. Krebs. 1974. Population cycles in rodents. Sci. Amer. 230:38-46. Naylor, Alfred F. 1959. An experimental analysis of dispersal in the flour beetle, Tribolium confusum. Ecology 40:453-465. Naylor, Alfred F. 1961. Dispersal in the red flour beetle Tribolium castaneum (Tenebrionidae). Ecology 42(2):231-237. Naylor, Alfred F. 1965. Dispersal responses of female flour beetles, Tribolium confusum, to presence of larvae. Ecology 46: 341-343. Niemierko, W. and S. Cepelewicz. 1950. Studies in the biochemistry of the wax moth (Galleria mellonella). 1. Growth of the larvae and their chemical composition. Acta Biol. Exp. 15(4):57-68. Niemierko, W. and P. Wlodawer. 1950. ...2. Utilization of wax constituents by the larvae. Acta Biol. Exp. lS(5):69-78. Niemierko, W. and P. Wlodawer. 1952. ...7. The digestion of wax and utilization of unsaponifiable substances by larvae. Acta Biol. Exp. 16(12):157-l70. Niemierko, W. and L. Wojtczak. 1950. 3. Oxygen consumption of the larvae during starvation. Acta Biol. Exp. 15(6):79-90. Oertel, E. 1962. Behavior studies of the greater wax moth. XI Intern. Kongress fur. Entomologie, Wien. 532-536. Oertel, E. 1963. Greater wax moth develops on bumble bee cells. J. Econ. Ent. 56(4):543-544. Ogden, John C. 1969. Effect of components of conditioned medium on behavior in Tribolium confusum. Physiol. Zool. 42:266-274. Ogden, John C. 1970. Aspects of dispersal in Tribolium flour beetles. Physiol. Zool. 43:124-131. Ogden, John C. 1970b. Artificial selection for dispersal in flour beetles (Tenebrionidae: Tribolium). Ecology 51:130-133. Orgain, H. and M. W. Schein. 1953. A preliminary analysis of the physical environment of the Norway rat. Ecology 34:467-473. 304 Paddock, F. B. 1913. The life history and control of the bee-moth or wax-worm. Texas Agri. Exper. Station Bull. 158:15-30. Paddock, F. B. 1930. The bee moths. J. Econ. Ent. 23:422-428. Park, Thomas. 1932. Studies in population physiology. The relation of numbers to initial population growth in the flour beetle Tribolium confusum. Ecology 13:172-181. Park, Thomas. 1933. ...11. Factors regulating initial growth of Tribolium confusum populations. J. Exp. Zool. 65:17-42. Park, Thomas. 1934. ...III. The effect of conditioned flour upon the productivity and population decline of Tribolium confusum. J. Exp. Zool. 68(2):l67-182. Park, Thomas. 1935. ...IV. Some physiological effects of conditioned flour upon Tribolium confusum Duval and its populations. Physiol. Zool. 8:91-115. Park, Thomas. 1936a. ...V. The oxygen consumption of the flour beetle, Tribolium confusum Duval. J. of Cellular and Comp. Physiol. 7(3):3l3-323. Park, Thomas. 1936b. ...VI. The effect of differentially conditioned flour upon the fecundity and fertility of Tribolium confusum Duval. J. Exp. 2001. 73(3):393-404. Park, Thomas and Woollcott. 1937. ...VII. The relation of environ- mental conditioning to the decline of Tribolium confusum populations. Physiol. Zool. 10:197-211. Park, Thomas. 1938. Studies in population physiology. VIII. The effects of larval population density on the post-embryonic development of the flour beetle T. confusum. J. Exp. Zool. 79:51-70. '- Park, Thomas, E. V. Miller, and C. 2. Lutherman. 1939. ...IX. The effect of imago population density on the duration of the larval and pupal stages of Tribolium confusum Duval. Ecology 20:365-373. Park, Thomas and W. Burrows. 1942. The reproduction of Tribolium confusum Duval in a semisynthetic wooddust medium. Physiol. Zool. 15:476-484. Park, Thomas. 1948. Experimental studies of interspecies competition. 1. Competition between populations of the flour beetles, Tribolium confusum Duval and Tribolium castaneum Herbst. Ecol. Monogr. 18:265-307. 305 Parker, J. S. 1930. Some effects of temperature and moisture upon Melanoplus mexicanus saussure and Camnula pellucida Scudder (Orthoptera). Univ. Montana Agri. Expt. Sta. Bull. No. 223. Pearl, Raymond. 1927. The growth of populations. Quart. Rev. Biol. 2:530-548. Peebles, Florence. 1929. Growth regulating substances in echinoderm larvae. Biol. Bull. 57:176-187. Pfeiffer, W. 1963. Alarm substances. Experientia. 19:113-168. Pipa, R. L. 1963. Studies on the hexapod nervous system. VI. Ventral nerve cord shortening; a metamorphic process in Galleria mellonella (L.) (Lepidoptera, Pyrallidae). Biol. Bull. Woods Hole. 124:293-302. Plath, O. E. 1934. Bumblebees and Their Ways. New York: Macmillan. Prus, T. 1963. Search for methods to investigate mobility in Tribolium. Ecology 44:801-803. Przelecka, A. 1956. Studies on the biochemistry of wax moth (Galleria mellonella L.). 14. Cytochemical study of phos- pholipids in the intestinal tract of wax moth larvae. Acta Biol. Exp. l7(l):23l-234. Rathcke, Beverly J. and Robert W. Poole. 1975. Coevolutionary race continues: butterfly larval adaptation to plant trichomes. Science 187:175-176. Regnies, Fred E. and John H. Law. 1968. Insect phermones. J. of Lipid Res. 9:541-551. Reynierse, James H. 1967. Aggregation formation in planaria, Phagocata gracilis and Cura foremani: species differentiation. Anim. Behav. 15:270-272. Reynierse, James H., Kathryn K. Gleason, and Robert Ottermann. 1969. Mechanisms producing aggregations in planaria. Anim. Behav. l7(l):47-63. Rich, Earl R. 1956. Egg cannibalism and fecundity in Tribolium. Ecology 37:109-120. Richards, C. M. 1958. The inhibition of growth in crowded Rana pipiens tadpoles. Physiol. 2001. 31:138-151. Robertson, T. B. 1921. Experimental studies on cellular manipulation. II. The influence of mutual antiquity upon reproductive rate and the part played therein by the "x-substance" in bacterized infusions which stimulates the multiplication of infusoria. Biochem. Jour. 15:595-611, 612-619. 306 Rohlf, James F. and Robert R. Sokal. 1969. Statistical Tables. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company. Rose, S. Meryl. 1959. Failure of survival of slowly growing members of a population. Science 129:1026. Rose, S. M. 1960. A feedback mechanism of growth control in tadpoles. Ecology 41:188-199. Roth, Louis M. and Ruth B. Howland. 1941. Studies on the gaseous secretion of Tribolium confusum Duval. I. Abnormalities produced in Tribolium confusum Duval by exposure to a secretion given off by the adults. Ann. Ent. Soc. Am. 34(1):151-l75. Roth, Louis M. 1943. Studies on the gaseous secretion of Tribolium confusum Duval. II. The odoriferous glands of Tribolium confusum. Ent. Soc. of Amer., Annals 36:397-424. Salt, George and F. S. J. Hollick. 1946. Studies of wireworm popu- lations. 11. Spatial distribution. J. Exp. Biol. 23:1-46. Sharp, David T. and I. B. Gray. 1962. Studies on factors affecting the local distribution of two sea urchins, Arbacia punctulata and Lytechinus variegatus. Ecology 43:309-313. Shaw, Gretchen. 1932. The effect of biologically conditioned water upon rate of growth in fishes and amphibia. Ecology 13: 263-278. Shelford, V. E. 1913. The reactions of certain animals to gradients of evaporating power of air. A study in experimental ecology. Biol. Bull. 25:70-120. Silverstein, Robert M., Otto J. Rodin, Wendell E. Burkholder, and John E. Gorman. 1967. Sex attractant of the black carpet beetle. Science 157:85-86. Silverstein, Robert M., Robert G. Brownlee, Thomas E. Bellas, David L. Wood, and Lloyd E. Browne. 1967. Brevicomin: Principal sex attractant in the frass of the female pine beetle. Science 159:889-890. Skaife, S. H. 1954. African Insect Life. London: Longmans Green. Sladen, F. W. L. 1912. The Bumble-Bee. London: Macmillan. Smith, G. 1915. The life-cycle of Cladocera, with remarks on the physiology of growth and reproduction in crustacea. Proc. Roy. Soc. London B. 88:418-535. Smith, Stephen D. 1969. The effects of crowding on larvae of the meal moth, Ephestia kuhniella. J. Exp. Zool. 170:193-203. 307 Skal, Robert R. and James F. Rohlf. 1969. Biometry. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company. Sokoloff, A. 1966. A behavioral difference of two mutant strains in Tribolium castaneum. Tribolium Inform. Bull. 9:109-110. Stanley, J. 1932. A mathematical theory of the growth of populations of the flour beetle Tribolium confusum Duval. Can. J. Research 6:632-671. Stanley, John. 1949. A mathematical theory of the growth of popu- lations of the flour beetle, Tribolium confusum Duv. VII. A study of the "Re-tunneling Problem." Ecology 30(2): 209-222. Stickle, L. F. 1946. The source of animals moving into a depopu- lated area. J. Mamm. 27:301-307. Strecker, R. L. 1954. Regulatory mechanics in house mouse popu- lations--the effect of limited food supply on a confined population. Ecology 34:375-385. Stuart, C. A. and A. M. Banta. 1931. Available bacteria and the sex ratio in Moina. Physiol. Zool. 4:72-86. Surtees, Gordon. 1963a. Laboratory studies on dispersion behavior of adult beetles in grain. I. The grain weevil, Sitophilus granarius (L.) (Coleoptera, Curculionidae). Bull. Ent. Res. 54:147-159. Surtees, Gordon. 1963b. ...II. The saw-toothed grain beetle, Oryzaephilus surinamensis (L.) (Coleoptera, Silvanidae). Bull. Ent. Res. 54:285-296. Surtees, Gordon. 1963c. ...III. Tribolium castaneum (Hbst.) (Coleoptera, Tenebrionidae) and ngptolestes ferrugineus 4 (Steph.) (Coleoptera, Cucujidae). Bull. Ent. Res. 54:297-306. Surtees, Gordon. l964a. ...VI. Three-dimensional analysis of dis- persion of five species in a uniform bulk. Bull. Ent. Res. 55:161-171. Surtees, Gordon. 1964b. ...VII. The effect of isolated pockets of damp and mouldy grain on Tribolium castaneum (Hbst.) (Coleoptera, Tenebrionidae). Bull. Ent. Res. 55:327-337. Surtees, Gordon. 1964c. ...VIII. Spontaneous activity in three species and a new approach to analysis of kinesis mechanisms. Anim. Behav. 12:374-377. Surtees, Gordon. 1964d. ...IX. Some differences in behavior of mated and unmated grain weevils. Entom. Monthly Mag. 100: 105-108. 308 Surtees, Gordon. 1964e. ...XI. Some effects of temperature. Anim. Behav. 12:378-381. Surtees, Gordon. l964f. ...XII. The effect of isolated pockets of damp and mouldy wheat on Cryptolestes ferrugineus (Steph.) (Coleoptera, Cucujidae). Bull. Ent. Res. 55:673-680. Talbot, Mayr. 1934. Distribution of ant species in the Chicago region with reference to ecological factors and physiological toleration. Ecology 15:416-439. Taylor, Sneyd J. 1931. A case of "assembling" in the phasmidae. Proc. Ent. Soc. London. 6:45. Terman, C. Richard. 1961. Some dynamics of spatial distribution within semi-natural populations of prairie deermice. Ecology 42:288-302. Thorpe, W. H., A. C. Crombie, R. Hill, and J. H. Darrah. 1946. The behavior of wireworms in response to chemical stimulation. J. Exp. Biol. 23:234-267. Uvarov, B. P. 1928. Grasshoppers and Locusts. London: Imp. Bur. Entom. 352 pp. Van der Kloot, William G. and Carroll M. Williams. 1953. Cocoon construction by the Cecropia silkworm. I. The role of the external environment. Behaviour 2+3zl4l-156. Van der Kloot, William G. and Carroll M. Williams. 1953b. ...II. The role of the internal environment. Behaviour 5:157-174. von Frisch, Karl. 1974. ’Animal Architecture. New York and London: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Walrecht, B. J., Jr. 1953. Similarity of the building instinct of social bees and wasps. Levende Natur. 61(3):70-72. Walter, H. E. 1907. The reactions of planaria to light. J. Exp. Zool. 5:35-162. Warburg, M. R. 1964. The response of isopods towards temperature, humidity, and light. Anim. Behav. 12:175-186. Waterhouse. 1959. Axenic culture of wax moths for digestion studies. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 77:283-289. Watson, H. S. 1953. Serial correlation in regression analysis. I. Biometrika. 42:327-341. 309 Way, M. J. and M. Cammell. 1970. Aggregation behavior in relation to food utilization by aphids. In Animal Populations in Relation to Their Food Resources (ed. Adam Watson). 1970. Bkackwell Scientific Publ. Oxford and Edinburgh. Wecker, Stanley C. 1963. The role of early experience in habitat selection by the prairie deer mouse, Peromyscus maniculatus bairdi. Ecol. Monogr. 33:307-325. Wellington, W. G. 1948. The light reactions of the spruce budworm, Choristoneura fumiferana Clemens (Lepidoptera: Tortruidae). Canad. Ent. 80:56-82. Wellington, W. G. 1957. Individual differences as a factor in population dynamics: the development of a problem. Canad. J. Zool. 35:293-323. Wellington, W. G. 1960. Qualitative changes in natural populations during changes in abundance. Canad. J. Zool. 38:289-314. Welty, J. C. 1934. Experimental explorations into group behavior of fishes: a study of the influence of the group on individual behavior. Physiol. Zool. 7. West, L. B. 1961. The nature of growth inhibitory material from crowded Rana pipiens tadpoles. Physiol. Zool. 33:232-239.- Whitcomb, Warren. 1936. The wax moth and its control. U.S. Dept. of Agri. Circular No. 386:1-10. Whitcomb, Warren. 1942. The wax moth and its control. U.S. Dept. of Agri. Circular No. 386:1-11. Williams, 0. L. 1934. Chapter 4, Termites and Termite Control. University of California Press. Berkeley, California. Witt, Peter N. and Charles F. Reed. 1965. Spider-web building. Measurement of web geometry identifies components in a com- plex invertebrate behavior pattern. Science 149:1190-1197. Wojtczak, L. 1952a. Studies on the biochemistry of the wax moth (Galleria mellonella). 10. Respiratory enzymes of the larva. Acta Biol. Exp. 16(15):199-222. Wojtczak, L. 1952b. ...ll. Respiratory enzymes in development. Acta Biol. Exp. l6(l6):223-238. Wojtezak, L. 1952c. ...12. Respiratory enzymes of the larva during starvation. Acta Biol. Exp. l6(l7):239-246. 310 Woolever, Patricia and Rudolph Pipa. 1970. Spatial and feeding requirements for pupation of last instar larval Galleria mellonella Lepidoptera. J. Insect Physiol. 16:251-262. Wynne-Edwards, V. C. 1962. Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behavior. Edinburgh and London: Oliver and Boyd. Young, Roger G. 1961. The effects of dietary beeswax and wax com— ponents on the larvae of the greater wax moth, Galleria mellonella (L.). Ann. Entom. Soc. Amer. 54:657-659. Young, Roger G. 1964. Digestion of wax by the greater wax moth, Galleria mellonella. Ann. Entom. Soc. Amer. 57:325-327. Zielinska, A. M. 1952. Studies on the biochemistry of the wax moth (Galleria mellonella). 8. Nitrogen metabolism of the larvae. Acta Biol. Exper. 16(13):171-186. Zromska-Rudyka, H. 1966. An appreciation of the laboratory set for investigating the emigratory activity of Tribolium. Ekologic Polska (A) 14:125-132.