pg. -.......‘-. .‘ ATE RSITY LIBRARIES lilil‘lllll Will 1 1,1 I‘ 3 1293 [LOL76 15H81 This is to certify that the thesis entitled Culture, Control, and Coping: The New Social Support Model presented by Belle Liang has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for #M A degree in Jiinicaleychology J #— V w v I Major professor Date leiqiq' 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LtBHARY Michigan State- University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE .-,- lave} "=3 12 2000 ‘- ' [-'. \, T b ' i r J 1“ U " b1 9;) c. 0: r4“; 00-n- ll l fif {MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution cMWMJ-oi CULTURE, CONTROL. AND COPIN G: THE NEW SOCIAL SUPPORT MODEL By Belle Liang A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1991 ABSTRACT CULTURE, CONTROL, AND COPING: THE NEW SOCIAL SUPPORT MODEL By Belle Liang Researchers contend that whether social support buffers stress or provides direct benefits depends on how support is measured, as well as specific person variables (e.g., locus of control). It is not known whether current research findings generalize to cultures other than Anglo-Americans. This study integrates the issues of measure type, personality, and cultural influence in a comprehensive comparison of support utilization among Anglo-Americans and Chinese nationals. Measures appropriate for testing the stress-buffering model of social support were given to 198 students in a Midwestern university and 200 students in mainland China. Both measure type and locus of control orientation mediated the process of support utilization in each culture, but not in the same manner across cultures. For Anglos, stress-buffering effects of both perceived and received support were found only with internals. For Chinese, direct effects and a buffering pattern from perceived support were found with Chinese externals but not Chinese internals. The received support measure yielded negative buffering effects with the latter culture. To My Grandmother-- who inspires my faith and love of culture ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My sincerest gratitude and affection are due Professor Anne Bogat - advisor, critic, colleague, and friend. Also, I appreciate the guidance and encouragement of my other Thesis Committee members, Professors Bob Caldwell and Dozier Thornton. I owe a special thanks to Dr. Gordon Wood for his generous financial support and to my faithful and exhausted research assistants and language translators for their perseverance and humor. Finally, to my parents and brothers, who are a testament of \ the power of family, w?) ai ni. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ........................................... vii-viii LIST OF FIGURES .............................................. ix INTRODUCTION ............................................... 1 Social Support: Stress-Buffering or Main Effect? ..................... 1 Ethnicity and Support Process .................................. 3 Chinese Support Resources ............................... 4 Chinese Support Process ................................. 6 Measurement of Buffering Effects .......................... 7 Locus of Control and Support Utilization .......................... 8 Support Availability vs. Utilization ......................... .9 Chinese Locus of Control ............................... ll Locus of Control and Social Support by Stress Interaction .............. 13 Support and Network Scales ............................. 14 Measurement of Stress ................................ 15 Adjustment Index ..................................... 15 Locus of Control Measurement ........................... l6 Hypotheses .............................................. 16 METHOD .................................................... 18 Participants .............................................. 18 Measures ................................................ 18 Procedure ............................................... 22 RESULTS .................................................... 24 Univariate Between Group Comparisons .......................... 24 Multivariate Within Group Analyses ............................. 30 DISCUSSION ................................................. 51 Limitations of the Study ..................................... 55 Sample Biases ...................................... 55 Measmement Biases .................................. 56 Future Directions .......................................... 59 APPENDICES ................................................. 61 A: Internality, Powerful Others, Control Scale ................... 62-63 B: Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviors .................. 64—65 C: Interpersonal Support Evaluation List ....................... 66-67 D: Social Relationship Chart ............................... 68-69 E: Density Chart .......................................... 70 F: Hassles Scale ....................................... 71-72 G: General Health Questionnaire ........................... 73-76 H: Consent Form ......................................... ‘77 REFERENCES .............................................. 78-87 LIST OF TABLES Table ....................................................... Page 1. Pearson Correlations for US. Sample ........................ 25 II. Pearson Correlations for Chinese Sample ....................... 26 III. Descriptive Statistics of Key Variables ....................... 28 IV. Separate Two-Way Anova’s for Key Variables ................ 31-32 V. Hierarchical Regression With ISSB Predicting to Adjustment: Anglos ......................... 36 VI. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Composite Predicting to Adjustment: Anglos ......................... 37 VII. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Appraisal Predicting to Adjustment: Anglos ......................... 38 VIII. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL—Esteem Predicting to Adjustment: Anglos ......................... 39 IX. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Tangible Predicting to Adjustment: Anglos ......................... 40 X. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Belonging Predicting to Adjustment: Anglos ......................... 41 XI. Hierarchical Regression With ISSB Predicting to Adjustment: Chinese . .42 XII. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Composite Predicting to Adjustment: Chinese ......................... 43 XIII. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Appraisal Predicting to Adjustment: Chinese ......................... 44 XIV. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Esteem Predicting to Adjustment: Chinese ......................... 45 XV. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Tangible Predicting to Adjustment: Chinese ......................... 46 XVI. Hierarchical Regression With ISEL-Belonging Predicting to Adjustment: Chinese ......................... 47 LIST OF FIGURES Figure ...................................................... Page 1. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISSB: Anglo Externals ...................... 34 2. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISSB: Anglo Internals ....................... 35 3. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISSB: Chinese Extemals ..................... 49 4. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISEL: Chinese Externals .................... 50 5. The New Social Support Model ............................. 56 INTRODUCTION Virtually every segment of the American population is touched by stress-inducing life experiences. Broadly defined, a stressful event is one that requires some adaptation on the part of the individual, and may be one that requires greater emotional, physical, or material resources than the individual generally possesses (cf., Dowrenwend & Dowrenwend, 1974; Seyle, 1956). Researchers now acknowledge, however, that the effect of stressful life events on adjustment, life satisfaction, and overall health is not invariant or universal. Gauging every individual’s risk for disorder strictly by his or her level of exposure to stressful life events is an over-simplification. Rather, individuals invoke varying resources and behaviors that influence the actual outcome of adversity. Hence, the inquiry regarding psychological and physiological outcomes of stressful life events has evolved into a more complicated discussion of psychosocial mediators. Social suppo_r_t: stress-buffering or main effect? Much of this discussion has centered on social support as a stress-buffer. Specifically, the perceived or actual functional and/or expressive provisions rendered 2 by formal and informal social relationships have been thought to alleviate the potentially harmful effects of a significant yet undesirable life event. However, scholarly efforts to elucidate and generalize the buffering effects of social support have been frustrated by inconsistent findings across populations. While some studies have shown that support moderates the impact of stressful circumstances (e.g., Caplan, 1974; Cohen & McKay, 1984; Eaton, 1978; Gore, 1981), others fail to find buffering effects and demonstrate that social resources have an overall beneficial effect ("main effect"), irrespective of stress level (Cohen, Struening, Muhlin, Genevie, Kaplan, & Peck, 1982; Lin, Simeone, Ensel, & Kuo, 1979; Williams, Ware, & Donald, 1981). In an oft-cited review of the literature, Cohen and Wills (1985) acknowledge evidence for both processes. They contend that the detection of either buffering or direct effects depends on the type of support, and ultimately, the mode of measurement; that is, whether social network structure versus function is assessed, or whether the measure assesses a specific structure/function versus combining several structural/functional measures into an undifferentiated global index. Buffering effects are expected when researchers employ specific functional support scales which measure and find functional support relevant to the recipient’s needs. For instance, the ISEL (Cohen & Hoberman, 1983) provides four separate scores that assess the availability of (1) esteem, (2) tangible, (3) social companionship or belongingness, and (4) appraisal or informational support. The ISSB (Banera, Sandler, & Ramsey, 1981), which assesses actual receipt of social support, is also a functional type measure. 3 While specific functional measures show more interaction effects, global structural measures better detect main effects. The latter combine a variety of items about connections with neighbors, relatives, and community organizations (e.g., frequency of talldng with friends/neighbors, degree of neighborhood cohesion). Hence, global structural scales indicate social integration (i.e., large number of relationships, embeddedness, and high network density); more socially integrated individuals experience greater direct effects from support resources. Specific structural measures that provide a quantitative count of social connections (e. g., number of nearby relatives) typically do not show significant main effects unless they index the presence of a significant interpersonal relationship (e.g., confidant measures), in which case they may also yield buffering effects. Ethnicig and sum process Although this formulation seems sufficient to explain the presence and absence of buffering effects with Anglo-American populations, whether the theory holds true across cultures remains a question. In particular, the present study examines the stress-mediating effects of social support among Chinese nationals. In their review, Cohen and Wills (1985) included only one study of an ethnic (Chinese-American) population (Lin et al., 1979). The lack of buffering effects detected in this study (Lin et al., 1979) was attributed to the use of a "global," rather than a "specific and appropriate" functional measure. In actuality, without fru'ther evidence, one cannot conclude whether the lack of buffering effects is an artifact of the support measure or is related to ethnic factors. For instance, it may be that 4 Chinese populations are less inclined than Anglos to utilize their social support in times of difficulty. This latter interpretation would be corroborated if, in a comparative study, Chinese indicated a lack of (or weak) buffering effects vis-a-vis Anglos, despite comparable levels of functional support resources. In fact, there is some evidence suggesting that precisely such results are likely. Chinese support resources. While research explicitly comparing social networks and support between Chinese and An glo-American populations is virtually nonexistent, a number of inferences regarding social support and network differences between these focal populations may be deduced from the related literature. First, the emphasis on collectivism rather than individualism (Hsu, 1953; Hwang, 1982; Kuo & Specs, 1983; Nuttal, Chieh, & Nuttall, 1988; Yang, 1981; Yu, 1980) and traditional Asian values have promoted strong extended family ties within a system of mutual obligation in China (Bengtson, 1968; Chang, Chang, & Shen, 1984; Hsu, 1953; Nuttall et al., 1988). In addition, lower levels of modernization, technology, and subsequent decreases in mobility among Chinese contribute to the establishment of geographically proximate social networks, which, in turn, lead to greater network density and multiple-roled relationships. Conversely, the greater mobility of American populations leads to geographically dispersed networks, limited to immediate kin, and an increase in other social connections. Based on these observations, Chinese populations should exhibit smaller networks of greater density, a greater proportion of family members, and more multiple-roled relationships. 5 Determining the extent to which these relationships may provide particular support functions, entails moving beyond structural assessments that merely describe the existence of relationships. Hence, researchers have included the categories of "source" (e.g., kin, friend, community) and "content" (e.g., instrumental or emotional) of support into their analyses. Especially relevant in the comparison of Chinese and American networks is the differentiation of generalist and specialist support. Support generalists--versatile network members who provide multiple forms of support--render different outcomes than support specialists who offer only a limited type of support. Bogat, Caldwell, Rogosch, and Kriegler (1985) suggest that network size has less influence on satisfaction than the consistency with which network members yield support (i.e., support generalists). In their study, family members were found to be the most consistent individuals in networks and thus represented support generalists: Further, individuals whose networks consisted of a larger proportion of family members reported greater satisfaction with support. This research lends evidence to the importance of "multidimensionality," or the quantity of functions served by a relationship (Mitchell & Trickett, 1980). The role of a network member may range from strictly a coworker to a combination of functions, such as tennis partner, confidante, classmate, and roommate. Studies assessing the networks of college students (Hirsch, 1979, 1980) and parents (Cochran & Brassard, 1979) have found multidimensional relationships to be more satisfying and stress-buffering. Relationships of this type demonstrate how a few quality friends and 6 relatives potentially supply more adequate and functional support than a large network of superficial relationships. Therefore, according to specialist-generalist and multidimensionality research, Chinese, whose networks are expected to consist of a greater proportion of family members and more multiple-roled relationships, should also have greater access to functional support than Anglo-Americans. Hence, according to Cohen and Wills’ (1985) formulation, if both populations were administered a specific functional measure, buffering effects comparable to or greater than those detected for Anglos should also be detected for Chinese. 9 Chinese support process. Interestingly, research employing Chinese (Chan, 1986; Lin et. a1, 1979) and other Asian populations (Aldwin & Greenberger, 1987; Graves & Graves, 1985; Uomoto, 1983; cited by Vaux, 1985) does not indicate that tight-knit ethnic families provide more stress-buffering social support for their members, nor that Asian college students provide higher levels of support to each other. In fact, Lin et a1. (1979) failed to find a significant buffering effect between stressors and illness for Chinese-American social support. Similarly, no buffering effects from social support were detected for a Polynesian population (Graves & Graves, 1985). Further, increased perceived crisis support not only did not buffer the effects of stressful life events on the physical symptoms of Hang—Kong Chinese, but even elevated psychological symptoms (Chan, 1986). Korean and Caucasian college students showed no difference in level of parental support, and Korean students reported confiding less often in their peers and receiving much less support from them 7 than did Anglo—American students (Aldwin & Greenberger, 1987). In a similar study, Asian-Americans reported fewer supportive behaviors from family and friends, and perceived their families as less supportive (Uomoto, 1983; cited by Vaux, 1985). Social support research with African-American populations may further corroborate these findings, since African-Americans generally share the Chinese affinity for large, close-knit family networks (Ball, Warheit, Vandiver, & Holzer, 1979, 1980; Cauce, Felner, & Primavera, 1982; McTavish, 1971; Raymond, Rhoads, & Raymond, 1980). Some research has demonstrated that low-income African-American women have similar friendship networks and larger family networks than poor Anglo women, but were less willing to utilize these resources during difficult circumstances (Ball et al., 1979, 1980). In another study (Stewart & Vaux, 1983), Anglo- and African-American college students reported remarkably similar support network resources (in terms of size, composition, and characteristics of relationships); yet African-American women reported friends as less supportive than did Anglo women. Measurement of bufi’ering efi'ects. All of these studies place in question the mediating effect of social support among Chinese and other ethnic groups possessing functionally resourceful networks. However, not one of these studies can wholly refute Cohen and Wills’ (1985) hypothesis on the relationship between functional support and stress-buffering, because none uses functional measures of support that Cohen and Wills (1985) have deemed "specific and appropriate." Instead, several studies employed structural support measures (Ball et al., 1979, 1980; Graves & Graves, 1985; Lin et al., 1979), and others utilized global functional measures (Chan 8 et al., 1986; Lin et al., 1979; Uomoto, 1983); neither of which are expected to detect significant buffering effects. Specifically, Lin et al.’s (1979) social support measrue consisted of a 9-item index that assessed feelings about neighborhood, frequency of talking with friends and neighbors, and involvement in the community. Chan (1986) employed a 5-item scale constructed by Andrews, Tennant, Hewson, and Vaillant (1978) termed crisis support, consisting of such questions as "In an emergency do you have friends/neighbors who would look after your family for a week?" and "If everything went badly, how many people could you turn to for comfort and support " Cohen and Wills (1985) categorize the former index as a "global structural support measure" and the latter as a "global functional support measure." A true test of Cohen and Wills’ (1985) stress-buffering hypothesis necessitates a comparative design where analogous Chinese and American populations are assessed by way of a "specific and appropriate functional measure." If, in this case, Chinese still do not evidence the interaction effects of stress and social support while Anglo-Americans do, the buffering hypothesis (Cohen & Wills, 1985) may require some revision to incorporate ethnic factors. Locus of control and suppgrt utilization The possibility that Cohen and Wills’ (1985) theory may not pertain to certain cultures is suggested by two studies (Lefcourt, Martin, & Saleh, 1984; Sandler & Lakey, 1982) which take into account personality differences between individuals and populations and their impact on social support. In bath studies, subjects were assessed for their locus of control and the effects of their social support. Although a functional 9 support measure was administered to all subjects (ISSB), buffering effects were found only with individuals having an internal locus of control. Cohen and Wills (1985) attempted to explain this discrepancy in buffering effects by suggesting that the ISSB "confounds the availability of support with the need for and use of support." However, they also contended that "work with compound but internally consistent functional measures provides evidence for the buffering model when measures assess perceived availability of support, but not when they assess use of support in the recent past." Hence, Cummins (1988) later replicawd the studies and appended an additional functional support measure (i.e., the Social . Provision Scale) which assessed the perceived availability of support. His results indicated that received social support (ISSB) buffers the effect of stress on symptoms for internals, just as was demonstrated in the previous research. Both perceived reassurance of worth (esteem support) and perceived guidance (appraisal support) failed to produce positive buffering effects for internals. , Support availability vs. utilization. Since only received social support buffered suess, these findings seem to indicate a distinction between the availability of functional support and its actual utilization. Ball et al.’s (1979, 1980) study comparing Anglo- and African-American low-income women corroborates this distinction through the use of a measure assessing the availability of support resources and respondents’ willingness to utilize them in times of difficulty. Their data indicate that, despite larger family networks and similar friendship networks, African-Americans are less willing to request help from family and friends than 10 Anglos. Because African-Americans generally indicate more external locus of control tendencies than Anglo-Americans (Levenson, 1974; Strickland, 1971; Sue, 1978), a plausible explanation for these results may be found in Sandler and Lakey’s (1982) contention that while externals amass more support connections, internals better utilize their available support. Based on Schacter’s (1959) study that demonstrates the tendency for individuals under stress to affiliate more, one might predict that externals would utilize more support than internals, since extemals report greater stress and anxiety (Lefcourt, 1976; Nelson & Phares, 1971, cited by Phares, 1976). However, Sandler and Lakey’s (1982) alternative view ensues from research demonstrating that although extemals experienced more negative feedback in response to threat, internals more actively sought resolution to the problem indicated by the feedback (Pth et al., 1968). The inclination to act on their own behalf may prompt internals to avail themselves of information; extemals have less need of information since they are more apt to rely on competent others. In one interesting example, involving hospitalized tuberculosis patients, internals exerted more effort in seeking out the cause and cure of their disease, and then attempted to act on this information (Seeman & Evans, 1962). Hence, based on a fund of literature indicating that internals are more active information-gatherers and more effective consumers of the available information (Lefcourt et al., 1973; Phares, 1968; Seeman, 1963; Strickland, 1978; Wolk & DuCette, 1974), researchers theorized that internals in locus of control would be more 11 likely to utilize informational support as an aid to coping with and diffusing stress (Lefcourt, Martin, & Saleh, 1984; Sandler & Lakey, 1982). Cummins (1988) also argued the likelihood of internals to derive greater benefit from esteem support, since internal attributions of negative events threaten self-esteem, and in turn, lead to depression (Peterson & Seligman, 1984, cited by Cummins, 1988). This deduction led Cummins (1988) to suggest that internals’ self-esteem support may act as a buffer from the possible depressive effects of negative events. Evidence that attests that internals are better able to utilize informational and esteem support is apropos because these two types of support are most broadly applicable to needs salient during times of stress, and thus, are most likely to produce buffering effects. Corroborative data for this expected relationship between locus of control and support utilization during stress was obtained when significant interactions between negative life events and social support in predicting measures for psychological disuess were detected for internals but not for externals. Chinese locus of control. The lack of buffering effects detected for Chinese, despite adequate functional support resources, is also consistent with Sandler and Lakey’s theory, since Chinese, as a whole, are more likely to evidence an external locus of connol. The earliest cross-cultural comparison of locus of conu'ol with Chinese subjects is that by Hsieh, Shybut, and Lotsof (1969). Based on Rotter’s (1966) LE scale, Hang-Kong Chinese indicated a greater belief in external control of reinforcement than 12 American-born Chinese, who in turn indicated greater external control beliefs than Anglo-Americans. Other studies (e.g., Tseng, 1972) have since corroborated the finding of higher externality among Chinese. These results seem consistent with research that purports a relationship between intemality and access to power, economic opportunity, social mobility, and individualistic ideology (Cook & Chi, 1984; Lefcourt, 1982; Phares, 1976; Sue, 1978). In a multidimensional analysis of Rotter’s I-E scale (Chan, 1989), Hong-Kong Chinese demonstrated greater extemality on a general luck (or fate) factor, but not on the achievement factor. Lao (1977) employed another multidimensional I-E Scale developed by Levenson (1974) to measure the belief in powerful others, the belief in internal control, and the belief in chance. Cross-cultural differences were found on all but the last factor. On the first factor, Chinese were found to hold stronger beliefs in powerful others than were Americans. Analysis of the second factor revealed differences across culture and sex, in that American females had a greater tendency to believe in internal events than Chinese females, while both male groups showed no significant difference. In another study (Lao, Chuang, & Yang, 1977), sex differences were found only for Chinese subjects. Compared to females, Chinese males indicated greater internal control on all three factors, and less conu'ol by chance. In contrast, American females did not score lower on internality than American males. Differences in locus of control between cultures have been attributed to varying degrees of modernity. For instance, level of modernity correlates negatively to 13 Rotter’s (1966) LE scale; the more modernized tend to attribute causality to greater personal effort, foresight, and acceptance of responsibility (Lefcourt, 1982). Further support to this theory is provided by the study of ’yuan’ (Yang, 1982). This exclusively Chinese concept represents a set of fatalistic beliefs in which almost every interpersonal relationship or transaction is largely predetermined by fate, some unknown force, or an individual’s conduct during a former life. In a large sample of Chinese students, those scoring high on individual modernity were less inclined to believe in ’yuan’. Since ’yuan’ exemplifies a type of external conu'ol belief, these results may be interpreted as indicating a weaker tendency among the more modernized to have external attributions. Locus of wand and social sum x stress interaction In view of explications evolving from locus of control research (Cummins, 1988; Lefcourt et al., 1984; Sandler & Lakey, 1982), Cohen and Wills’ (1985) formula for predicting social support’s buffering effects appears to be an oversimplification. Locus of control, as a moderator of social support’s buffering effects, may be a critical variable missing from their equation. Such a conclusion would be especially germane in comparisons between Chinese and Anglo-American populations, since both typically fall on opposite poles of the Internal-External Locus of Control continuum, Chinese tending toward externality, and Americans toward intemality. The lack of stress-buffering effects found in Chinese populations could then be attributed to externally-oriented individuals’ lesser utilization of functional support resources during times of difficulty. 14 However, studies which examine the influence of locus of control on stress and social support have yet to be replicated cross-culturally. By comparing data across two different cultures, Chinese and Anglo—American, the present study attempted to substantiate the cross-cultural applicability of Sandler and Lakey’s Locus of Control hypothesis. This study employed six measures: a daily hassles inventory, eight measures of social support, one of psychological adjustment, and a locus of control scale. Support and network scales. The ISSB was utilized in previous studies (Cummins, 1988; Lefcourt et al., 1984; Sandler & Lakey, 1982); the latest of which (Cummins, 1988) also included a measure of perceived availability of support, which Cohen and Wills suggest to be more appropriate for assessing the buffering hypothesis. Hence, the present study represents a replication and extension of these former studies by employing a three-part social support questionnaire. First, a global structural network measure was administered in order to assess multidimensionality, network density, and composition. As discussed previously, these measures were used to predict the potential availability of functional support. Second, the ISEL (Cohen & Hoberman, 1983), a "specific functional support measure" appropriate for testing the buffering hypothesis (Cohen & Wills, 1985), was used to assess the perceived availability of support. These two scales were administered in conjunction with the Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviors (ISSB) - a functional support measure requiring respondents to report on support received in the last month. Making this distinction between availability of and receipt of functional support is especially 15 apropos in the present study, because Chinese are predicted to have access to social support, but to be less likely to utilize or receive it during times of stress. Measurement of stress. Ethnic factors were also taken into account in the selection of a life stress measure. To date, "episodic" life events indices have been utilized extensively with ethnic populations following the seminal work of Holmes and Rahe (1967). These authors reported substantial agreement in the rating of life events across status groups differing in age, and in sociocultural and ethnic backgrounds. In a study explicitly comparing life event scaling between a Chinese and Anglo population, Chan, Chan-Ho, and Chan (1984) reported a general correspondence across cultures in the degree of distress experienced from negative events. Although less research has evaluated the cross-cultural validity of "ongoing" life events indices, the present study utilizes a "Daily Hassles and Uplifts" measure (abridged version of Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1981), because chronic sources of stress better predict psychological and somatic health-related outcomes (Burks & Martin, 1985; Del.ongis, Coyne, Dakof, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1962). Adjustment index. Traditionally, cross-cultural research indicated that Chinese commonly experienced distress in somatic forms. However, more recent research (e.g., Cheung, 1982, 1984; Kleinman, 1982) has suggested a distinction between symptom presentation and manifestation. While the majority of Chinese depressives initially presented with somatic complaints, when directly asked, most admitted having some form of dysphoria. Hence, the tendency of Chinese to evince distress through a mixture of affective and somatic complaints necessitates an adjustment measure that 16 assesses both psychological and physical symptomatology. The 60-item version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) was selected for its proven reliability and validity with Chinese populations (Chan, 1983; 1985), as well as Anglo populations (Vieweg & Hedlund, 1983). Locus of control measurement. A number of cross-cultural studies (Bond & Tomatzky, 1973; Hsieh et al., 1969; Mahler, 1974; Tseng, 1972) have utilized Rotter’s (1966) Internality-Externality scale to assess locus of come]. However, critics of the scale cast doubt on its validity with non-Anglo populations. First, contrary to Rotter’s findings with an Anglo-American population, separate factors were detected for . personal and general causality in the African-American sample. Taking into account political factions, it seems also feasible that mainland Chinese would reveal some disjunction between ways in which they perceive their own experience versus others’ of the "free worl " A second problem in the scale’s generalizability arises from the varying impacts of diverse controlling agents. For instance, "control by chance" and "control by powerful others" have often been regarded as separate factors. Levenson’s (197 3) efforts to remedy both forms of multidimensionality in Rotter’s (1966) LE scale, have produced a three-factor index termed "Internal, Powerful Others, and Chance" (IPC) which refers strictly to personal outcomes. Hymtheses Part (I): Univariate comparisons between populations. A. It was expected that Chinese, as compared to Anglos, would have higher scores on density, and family composition, and subsequently higher scores on perceived 17 availability of social support as measured by the ISEL-Composite and ISEL-subscales. B. Greater receipt of supportive behaviors as measured by the IS SB and higher intemality, was predicted for Anglos, relative to Chinese. Part (H): Multivariate comparisons within populations. A. For Anglo and Chinese internals, it was predicted that significant effects would be detected for the stress x social support terms (i.e., HASSLE-INTENSITY x ISSB, HASSLE-INTENSITY x ISEL-Composite or ISEL-subscales). B. For Anglo and Chinese externals, the stress x social support terms were expected to be nonsignificant. METHOD Participants Participants consisted of two samples of college students. The first group was comprised of 198 Anglo-American students (45% male, 55% female) attending a typical Midwestern university. The second group included 200 Chinese students (59% male, 41% female) from six universities/colleges in Beijing, China and five universities/colleges in Nanjing, China. Measures Levenson’s Multidimensional Locus of Control Scale (1981) was chosen to assess perceptions of control (see Appendix A for a copy of the scale). This 244nm index (three 8-item subscales) applies a 6-point Likert format (ranges from "strongly disagree" to "strongly agree") and differentiates three dimensions of conuol: Internality (1), Powerful Others (P), and Chance (C). All items are phrased in terms of personal outcomes, rather than a generalized world view. Reliability for the I, P, and C subscales ranges between .51-.67, .72-.82, and 18 19 .73-.79 respectively, with various populations (Levenson, 1973, 1974). In the present study, I, P, and C reliabilities were .57, .73, and .77, for Anglos, and .50, .48, and .42, for Chinese, respectively. Previous studies indicate that the correlation between the P and C scales tends to fall between .41-.60, while the correlation between these two subscales and the I subscale is only .19-.25 (Levenson, 1973). The present study obtained a measure of high and low intemality by analyzing only the Internality (I) subscale. The I factor is scored in the internal direction, whereas the P and C factors are scored in the external direction. The Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviors (ISSB; Barrera, Sandler, & Ramsey, 1981) measures the receipt of supportive behaviors (see Appendix B for a copy of the scale). In this 40-item, 5-point Likert scale (ranges from "not at all" to "about every day"), respondents report the frequency of occurrence in the past month of such support as "having had someone stay with you [the subject] in a stressful situation" and "having had someone provide you with information to help you understand your situation." Barrera (1981) reported test-retest correlation coefficients for individual items ranging from .44 to .91. Coefficient alphas of .93 and .94 confirmed the instrument’s high internal consistency; this justifies the use of total composite scores by summing frequency ratings across all forty items. (In the present study, the alpha coefficient of the ISSB was .94 for both the Chinese and Anglos.) Additionally, the ISSB was positively correlated with a qualitative index of support (i.e., perceived supportiveness). 20 The Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL--college student version; Cohen & Hoberman, 1983) assesses perceived availability of potential support resources (see Appendix C for a copy of the scale). This 48-item index differentiates four types of social support (12-items per subscale): ISEL-Tangible (material aid); ISEL-Appraisal" (help in defining, understanding, and coping with problematic circumstances); ISEL-Esteem (sources confirming one’s worth and acceptance by others); and ISEL-Belonging (companions to spend time with in leisure and recreational activities). The respondent answers "probably TRUE" or "probably FALSE" to items such as "There is really no one I can trust to give me good financial advice" and "Most people I know think highly of me." Cohen, Mermelstein, Kamarck, and Hoberman (1985) found that the ISEL is moderately correlated with the existing structural, past support, and perceived availability measures. Second, both the total scale and subscales have evidenced adequate internal and test-retest reliabilities in several samples. Furthermore, the ISEL’s discriminant validity has been demonstrated by its lack of correlation with the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability Scale and a social anxiety scale. In the present study, the alpha reliability for the ISEL-Composite was .86 for Anglos and .76 for Chinese. Alpha coefficients of ISEL-Tangible, ISEL-Belonging, ISEL-Appraisal, and ISEL-Esteem were .62, .67, .81, and .46 for Anglos, and .46, .63, .60., and .30 for Chinese, respectively. Social networks or potential support resources were measured by a density chart (Hirsch, 1979) and an assessment of multidimensional relationships developed 21 for this study (see Appendix D and E for copies of these instruments). Respondents were required to list up to 20 names of currently significant friends or relatives, including people with whom they interacted in a variety of activities (e.g., studying, going to the movies, or sharing personal concerns) on a regular basis (at least once during any 2- to 3- week period). Each of these relationships was placed in one or more of the following categories of support: self-esteem, appraisal, tangible assistance, and appraisal/informational. Density was calculated through the procedure outlined by Hirsch (1979). The Daily Hassles Scale (Kanner et al., 1980), in modified form, served as a chronic stress index (see Appendix F for a copy of this measure). Only those items germane to both Chinese and American cultures were included (e.g., "misplacing or losing things," "not getting along with a frien "). Less relevant items were discarded (e.g., "auto maintenance problems"). The respondent was instructed to check items, and suggest any other hassles, that have occurred in the past month. They also rated each hassle for severity (l=somewhat severe, 2=moderately severe, 3=extremely severe). For each subject, three summary scores were generated: (1) fiequency--a count of the number of checked items; (2) cumulated severity-—the sum of the 3-point severity ratings; and (3) intensity--the cumulated severity divided by the frequency. The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ; Goldberg, 1972) consists of 60 questions concerned with psychological distress or altered behavior (see Appendix G for a copy of this scale). For each item, respondents compared their recent state (past 22 month) with their usual state; only those symptoms experienced more than usual are scored. Sample items include, "Have you recently lost much sleep over worry" and "...tended to lose interest in your ordinary activities?" In a review of GHQ studies (Vieweg & Hedlund, 1983), internal consistency reliability for the GHQ was reported to range from .78 to .95, test-retest estimates ranged from .51 to .90, and concurrent validity coefficients ranged from .55 to .83. The Chinese version of the 60-item GHQ (Chan, 1985) has demonstrated comparable psychometric properties. In addition to high internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and concurrent validity, factor analyses have revealed overall correspondence of the factor structures for both versions of the GHQ. Data of the present study revealed an alpha coefficient of .95 for Chinese and .96 for Anglos. Procedure Each of the measures were transcribed into Chinese by a translator of mainland Chinese descent, except the GHQ which had previously been translated and used by Chan (e.g., 1983, 1985, 1986). In order to validate the Chinese versions, two fluently bilingual judges independently translated them back into English. These procedures confirmed that the questionnaires were accurately translated. Questionnaires were shipped to a primary contact person in Beijing and and another in Nanjing, China. Both contact persons independently elicited the cooperation of several faculty persons at each of six universities and colleges in Beijing and five universities and colleges in Nanjing. These faculty members announced the experiment to their classes and then distributed questionnaires to 23 students interested in participating. Another set of questionnaires was given to Anglo-American Introductory Psychology students at Michigan State University in exchange for extra course credit. After signing a consent form (see Appendix H), each respondent was required to complete, in an anonymous fashion, one of three randomly ordered questionnaires in his or her own native language. RESULTS Univariate Between Group Comparisons Preliminary analyses revealed several significant relationships between variables. These relationships can be seen on Tables I and II. For Anglos, internality was significantly correlated with adjustment (r = .22, p < .01), negative life events (r = .30, p < .01), ISEL-Appraisal (r = .32, p < .01), ISEL-Esteem (r = .38, p < .01), ISEL-Tangible (r = .32, p < .01), and ISEL-Belonging (r = .35, p < .01). For Chinese, internality was also significantly correlated to adjustment (r = .15, p < .05) and negative life events (r = .16, p < .05), but it was only significantly related to two perceived support subscales, ISEL-Appraisal (r = .16, p < .05) and ISEL-Tangible (r = .17, p < .05). For both Anglo-Americans and Chinese, internal locus of control individuals reported fewer negative symptoms and life events and more perceived support. Regarding the dependent measure, the GHQ was not only related to internality, but also overall perceived support (r = .40, p < .01 for Anglos; r = .24, p < .01 for Chinese) and negative life events (r = .58, p < .01 for Anglos; r = .35, p < .01 24 8. v o. 8. v a. -- “caaoemhma .o .89 -- osacfihma .m .8“. .na. .. Baum—Ame s .mnm. .48. .em. -- anagram o .48. .mom. a:. .Sm. -- mg .n .5: .8? 2a.. .8? LE. -- sea: .4 .30- .8? .8? .58.- ”8.- .oR. -- one .m an”. as. he. ex. «8. .8? 53.. -- 3.52... .N N8. 2:. so. .3. :2. So. .98. 48. $50 ._ o a a o m 4 m N 295% .m.D new mega—ebony =8th .— 93:. 25 8. v o. 3. v a. -- moeceomnma .o .42. -- ozacesma— .w .84. .wom. -- Begum—Amm— .4. .5“. .nR. o2. -- amaoofinmm— o .Em. .5. 2:. «so. -- mg .m m:.- .5; 87 an..- .mfi. -- .23: .4 o:.- .42.- .w4_.- .80 :b. .5. -- 96 .m 82. ea. 8o. .5. o8. 14.2.- .§.. -- gases .N o3. So. So. 8a.- o8. ms. .SN. .2? a".450 ._ m w A. c n v m N 295m 03:20 8m Seen—2.80 £5th .= «San. 26 27 for Chinese). Chinese and Anglos that indicated a greater number of symptoms, were lower in perceived support and higher in negative life events. For Chinese, the number of family members within an individual’s network was related to two types of functional support, the ISSB (r = .26, p < .01) and ISEL-Appraisal (r = .25, p < .05). Demographic differences between the Anglo and Chinese groups were assessed by two separate 2 x 2 analyses of variance for Age and Gender with one between- group factor (Culture). Significant differences were found for both Age [F(1, 334)=160.81, p < .001)] and Gender [F(1, 388)=7.10, p < .01)]. Chinese subjects M = 21.17 yrs.) were significantly older than Anglo-American subjects M = 19.77 yrs.). The Anglo sample consisted of more females (11 = 108) than males (n = 90) and the Chinese sample was comprised of more males (n = 112) than females (n = 79). In order to assess differences in social support level between the Chinese and Anglo samples, nine separate 2 x 2 x 2 analyses of variance with two between factors (Culture and Gender) were conducted on nine measures of social support as the dependent variables (NUMBER OF SUPPORTERS, KIN RATIO, DENSITY, ISSB, ISEL-Composite, ISEL-Appraisal, ISEL-Esteem, ISEL-Tangible, ISEL-Belonging). See Table III for means and standard deviations of these variables. Significant culture effects were detected for NUMBER OF SUPPORTERS [F(1, 386)=69.42, p < .001)], DENSITY [F(1, 297)=9.64, p < .005)], the ISSB [F(1, 384)=20.17, p < .001], ISEL- Composite [F(1, 385)=100.17, p < .001)], ISEL-Appraisal [F(1, 385)=43.59, p < .001)], ISEL-Esteem [F(1, 385)=31.32, p < .001)], ISEL-Tangible [F(1, 386)=82.92, p < .001)]. and ISEL-Belonging [F(1, 386)=69.99, p < .001)]. Anglos, relative to 28 «3 Re 28 me 86 N: Go :2 «season ama— .. So 84 N3 8.. 25 e: a... e: same? nma .. Ed ”2 43 ”2 43 of 43 8e 88am ama— .. 43 S; so we; 86 a: was Re Baa? nma .. 25 N3 25 .3 43 w: o; m: seasoo .59 one «2 3o 43 «.3 8n and _4.~ m2 2.“ 8.: 43. 8.2 $4 8.2 3.4 no: aoeooosm a n3 :5 mac «8 «do 3c 36 as £28 to 43 23 one no one “3 Rd 25 .5. 8d 23 Go and one 84 So So 8.35 NE. was 84 ”non o4.4 when 8.4 Rom £3825 N46 no; So 4.: woo 43 Se 2: one am 2 on 2 no. .2 em 2 mug—«Eon— 832 33:5”— 832 80:20 8&3. use; no. no 83% 2.3.8.8 .E 2%... 29 Chinese, reported more social network members M = 14.98, M = 12.28; respectively), network density M = .84, M = .72; respectively), actual receipt of support M = 2.54, M = 2.28; respectively), perceived appraisal support M = 1.83, M = 1.68; respectively), perceived esteem support M = 1.66, M = 1.58; respectively), perceived tangible support M = 1.85, M = 1.68; respectively), perceived belonging support M = 1.72, M = 1.54; respectively). KIN RATIO was not significantly different across W cultures. Significant gender differences were found for KIN RATIO [F(1, 297)=3.69, f p = .05], the ISSB [F(1, 384)=8.61, p < .005], and ISEL-Appraisal [F(1, 384)=5.59, p < .02]. Males for both groups, relative to females, indicated a smaller proportion of family members in their social networks M = .24 for males, M = .28 for females) and less actual receipt of support M = 2.33 for males, M = 2.49 for females), whereas females had more perceived appraisal support M = 1.78 for females; M = 1.73 for males). The Culture by Gender interaction was significant for ISEL-Appraisal [F(1, 386)=6.45, p < .02]. Anglo-American females M = 1.88) and Chinese males M = 1.68) perceived more availability of appraisal support than did Anglo males M = 1.77) and Chinese females M = 1.67). A 2 x 2 (Culture by Gender) analysis of variance was conducted to determine differences in internal locus of control. Findings revealed significant differences between cultures for the I scale [F(1, 371)=172.55, p <.001] -- Anglos were more internal than Chinese M = 36.74, M = 29.22; respectively). Significant gender differences were also found [F(1, 371)=5.51, p <.02]. Males indicated more internality that females M = 33.65, M = 64.61; respectively). Further, results indicated a 30 significant Culture by Gender interaction effect [F(1, 371)=5.80, p <.02]. Chinese males M = 30.58) were more internal than Chinese females M = 27.86), whereas Anglo males and females were not significantly different in their level of internality M = 36.72, M = 36.75; respectively). Although there was no significant culture difference for the GHQ, a significant gender difference was found [F(1, 369)=l8.63, p <.001]. Findings revealed a greater {- number of symptoms for females than for males in both cultures M = 1.95, M = 1.78; respectively). Finally, no significant main effects or interaction were detected for the stress variable (HASSLES-INTENSITY). Findings from these analyses of variance .can be found in Table IV. Multivariate Within Group Analyses The direct and stress-buffering effects of received and perceived support for internal and external individuals were tested through separate hierarchical multiple regressions for each of the four locus of control groups (Anglo-intemal, Anglo- extemal, Chinese-intemal, Chinese-extemal). Subjects were divided into these respective internal and external groups using the average of the I scale means from both populations as the mean split. In each of the regressions, Gender was entered as the first variable, the Stress term (HASSLE-INTENSITY) was entered next, Social Support (ISSB or ISEL-subscale) was entered third, and the product of the last two terms (HASSLE-INTENSITY x ISSB or ISEL-subscale) was entered as the fourth variable. A "main effect" for social support was indicated by significant effects for the ISSB and ISEL terms, whereas a "buffering" effect was evidenced by significant Table IV. Separate Two-Way Anova’s for Key Variables i Gender Culture (G) (C) (G X C) Within GHQ MS 2.69 0.05 0.01 0.14 F 18.63” 0.37 0.10 Internality MS 165.64 5183.43 174.31 30.04 F 5.51' 172.55” 5.80' Hassles MS 0.14 1.00 0.05 0.28 F 0.49 3.62 0.17 Kin Ratio MS 0.10 0.00 0.00 0.03 F 3.69“ 0.01 0.08 Density MS 0.12 0.94 0.17 0.10 F 1.24 9.31" 1.64 # Supporters MS 5.71 582.29 76.26 22.74 F 0.25 25.61” 3.35 Note. df = 1 for gender, culture, and g x c ‘p < .05. "p < .01. 31 Table IV. (cont’d). Gender Culture (G) (C) (G X C) Within ISSB MS 2.69 6.30 0.62 0.31 F 20.17” 8.61" 1.99 ISEL-Composite MS 0.04 2.09 0.01 0.02 F 1.79 100.17" 0.64 ISEL-Appraisal MS 0.29 2.23 0.33 0.05 F 5.59’ 43.59" 6.45’ ISEL-Esteem MS 0.00 0.66 0.00 0.02 F 0.05 31.32" 0.01 ISEL-Tangible MS 0.07 2.88 0.01 0.03 F 2.15 82.92” 0.18 ISEL-Belonging MS 0.02 3.09 0.00 0.04 F 0.37 69.99" 0.04 Note. df = l for gender, culture, and g x c ‘ p < .05. b p < .01. 32 33 effects for the interaction terms (I-lASSLE-INTENSITY x ISSB and HASSLE- INTENSITY x ISEL-subscale). Results for the regression analyses employing Anglo-American data are presented in Tables V, VI, VII, VIII, D(, and X. For this population, the ISSB X HASSLE-INTENSITY interaction was significant in the prediction of Adjustment (GHQ) only for internal subjects (AR2 = .04, AF = 9.96, p < .01), not externals (see Figures 1 and 2). Findings revealed that received social support (ISSB) buffers the effect of stress on adjustment for Anglo internals as expected. Further, main effects for received support were detected for only Anglo externals (AI?2 = .07, AF = 5.08, p < .01). Those individuals who report greater receipt of social support indicate fewer symptoms of negative adjustment, irrespective of suess level. Similarly, buffering effects of perceived social support were found only for Anglo internals (ISEL- Appraisal--AR2 =.042, AF = 10.17, p < .005; ISEL-Esteem--AR2 = .026, AF = 6.36, p < .05; ISEL-Tangible—-AR2 = .028, AF = 6.46, p < .05; and ISEL-Belonging--AR2 = .039, AF = 10.02, p < .002). Anglo internals also demonsu‘ated main effects for all four types of perceived support--ISEL-Appraisal (AR2 =.03, AF = 6.90, p < .01), ISEL- Esteem (AR2 = .052, AF = 12.18, p < .001), ISEL-Tangible (AR2 = .02, AF = 4.08, p < .05), and ISEL-Belonging (AR2 = .07, AF = 16.45, p < .001). Extemals showed main effects for only ISEL-Appraisal (AR2 = .08, AF = 6.03, p < .02) and ISEL-Esteem (AR2 = .07, AF = 5.77, p < .02). In comparison with the US. sample, Chinese data yielded strikingly dissimilar results (see Tables XI, XII, XIII, )GV, XV, XVI). The ISSB X HASSLE-INTENSITY 10- —-— HI SUPPORT (+1 sd) 9 ' —'r— MED SUPPORT (mean) ->lé- LO SUPPORT (.1 sd) 8... DIG) 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 HASSLES Score Figure 1. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISSB: Anglo Externals 34 10" —-— HI SUPPORT (+1 sd) ' —l—- MED SUPPORT (mean) ->K— to SUPPORT (.1 sd) DIG) or I O i l t t i I t 1 l O 1 2 3 4 6 7 8 9 1 O 5 HASSLES Score Figure 2. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISSB: Anglo Internals 35 .svo. .8. v o. .mm. H Mn. .N»... u 4% d: u 2 : .mfiuMdeuEéth. 8.7 43 .23 .44.4~ mma x seam 8...- :3 m2 .35 m2 who 48 .SS. .4~.o4 32.5 o; 43 .26 En nose :«EEQE So- 8... Se .91. m2 x see: one- So ace .32 m5 one n2 .3: .3: woes: woo mod .84 .84 base .mREme a «”2 "a a oases SEE ”80:53:24. 9 mega»..— mmfl 5;? commmucwom 302883: .> 035. 36 .8v:. .3 v a. on n mu. «4. n a: o: u 2 .. .3 u we .3 u E .R u 2. cam- 43 .3: .32 4mm: on sea: 3? 3o .3: .23 4mm. 3:. 42 .32. .434 sea: a; 4% 3n .2.“ _ .528 3.525 on: 85 :3 .3» 5mm: x .23: one. So .53 .32 own: one 3.. .3: .3: .23: 3e 3e .84 .34 house .m—dF—me n :2 "a a ozaea> mo_w:< u.=o:em=_.e< o: manage: exogenous—mm: .33 Same—mom 333889: .—> 035. 37 .3. v a . .3 v o . 3 n 8144. 0 Ne .84 u z .. on u .213 u E .R u 2. 3:- 48: .:.S .88 3.288333: x 8.3.: 2d. 85 8o .88 2383.22 3c 48 .38 .484 Sea: :5 So .26 .2.n .850 :«3525 85 3a 8.8 .22 3:38:82 x 8:2: 8.? 3o .32. .32 2.288453. 85 3o .3: .3: sea: 86 3o 8.4 .84 .880 .mBEExm a .54 "a a 22.29 mo_w=< 2:253:54 o. weuomfia Gamay—$3.5m: 5m? Swamp—mom ~830th .=> 035,—. 38 I— .8. vm. .8. v a. mm. H mm. W». n «k d: u 2 2 NM. u Mu. Q». n N-.~hu Z. mo.m- 86 bad .awSN 83.5359 X 8.3mm mud- mod .35 Beam 88.5559 and vmd awed. .vmdv 8.3mm 26 vod .26 .n~.m .8980 .255...— 2: Ed wad .2; Sneak—mm— X 3.3“: 56. Ed .28 $2: EooaNJmm— and mud .35 .3." g 8.3“: wmd mod bad 53. H.650 .m_an§m n .m< "a m .32.; mo_w=< u:o§«=€< Q wéfivfla EooaMJm—m— .23 asap—mom 782882: .—=> 035. 39 .33.. 3:: 3 "3.3 ".26: "2.. .3 "33"....Ru2. 37 3o .83 .83 oémcahda x 8.2.: Ed- 86 .8... .33 usagefimm. 3o .3 .3fi .39. .23.: 2... 2:. .3n .flw .850 :mafies 2: 3o .2 .3» 2.35...-qu x 8.3.: 26. 3o 3. .3” 235....qu one 28 .3: .3: 8:3: ”3 3o .8... .8... .850 .m—danxm— a $2 .2 m o§E> 8&5... 3:08.83... 9 manomnpa ozmwcahfimm— FEB commmvaom 3028805 .5 «35,—. 4O fin.» .. . . .- ii. .8. v5 .8. v... 3 u .8 .8. u E 8: u z .. 8m. "81% ".82"? 8.7 88 .88 .83 0385.38. x 8.8.: :8- 88 .8... .28 28.8.8.de 88 £8 .88 .88.. «2.8: 28 88 .28 .28 .850 :mfiEoEH 888 88 § .68 2885.-ng x .22.: £8- 88 82 .88 288.98% and mmd .36.: .mcg fl 8.883 88 88 .83 .88... 58:5 .885me n «Md "2 m oEwE> 88.2 855.33 e 88082.. 288.8-qu as, 88.283. 8.28282: 82 «3:. 4O .8. V... .8. v... .8 u 8. .3 u .2 8: n z .. 8.. "88.. u Eduz. 5.7 3.0 88.9 35.8 wEwcofim—Jmm— X 8.883 88. 88 .88. .38 8.82.8808. 88 88 .32. .88.. 55.: 8.8 .88 .28 .28 .850 $352:— 88 88 :8 .88 8.828-008. x .25.: £8. 88 8... .88 8.828.408. 88 88 .3: .3: 5.5.: 88 88 .8... .8... .850 .mREoim 8 .8 "a m 25.5 80—95. 3:253:08. 9 300%»... mamwcofimfim—m— .23 commmflmom 30288.85 .X 030,—. 41 .8. v... .8. v... .8. u .8 .8. u .0 .8 n 2 .. 8.. u .8 .3 u .2 82 u 2. 38 88 8.. 88 82 x 58...: 8.8 88 888 2.8 m8. :8 88 83 88 58...: :8 88 88 88 .850 3&8535 82 88 .38 .88 888. x 55.: 88- 888 88 .88 888. 88 :8 .858 .3: 5.38. :8. 88 .888 .88 .850 .2580?” 8 .88 .8 8 288.3 80:20 08253.8(. 9 9.00? M59 .23 80.89%...”— 8029885 in 938,—. 42 NE 55 38 5o . Ea x 8.25 mod 86 ”3 «3 4mg 55 86 SN «3 “2%: :d :8 v3 Ed .880 :2an5- Va? 8o 86 a»? .5— x 3%: 8.? 35 a3 .32 4mm— »? Ed .33 ‘3: 828: ad mod .86 .36 350 .mEEoEm n «”2 "a n. 2.3:; 80:20 Hangman}. 9 wave? camanoUJmm— SS, gammy—wow 3058805 fix 935. 43 .8. v a. .8. v a. 3 "mug. "kahuz: .3. u 313 u E5: u 2. 2.? 5o .3 c2 Eaaafaa x .22.: :5 86 m2 9; 3.3.3.43. «Nd mod wed 9..— . 3333 25 So «no :3 .850 .2582:— 3. 8... Ed .8... unsaniémm— x .225 on? 36 53 .35 Eangma ”no Ed .33 .83 3...“: 5. 3o .03 .8... .850 .mRESxm n R< .2 m 03...; .856 355.3%... 9 $3062.. Exaggfima .23 8&2me 302080.: .Ex 2%... Enoamfim—m— x .2883 3... 8... 9... 8.. 8.? 8... .8 8... 8.2.98... .8... 8... 8.8 8.. 8...... 8... a... 3... 9.... .880 332.52.: R? 8... 8.... .8... 88.84.... x 8...... 2.? :8 8.. .88. 83.8%... 8... 3... .588 .8... 8...... .8 8... .88 .8... .850 .mRESxm a .8 .2. .. 03...; 80.....0 . 2258...... 9 «58...... 88.8.58 a... 88.2»... mango... Ex 2...... 45 .8. v... .8. v... 8. u .818. u .m .8 u 2.. 8.. u 818. u .2 .8... u 2 . 8.. 88 8.... 888 8.8.88.8. x 8...... 88 88 8.8 88 83.8.5.8. 88 88 8.. a... 8...... :8 88 88 .88 888.. :2an3— 8.? 88 88 .8... 8.8.88.8. x 8...... 8.? 88 .88 .8... 0.8.8.8.... 88 :8 .8... .8... 8...... 88 88 .88 .88 8880 .m_8Eo.xm 8 w... .8 .. 83...; 80.....0 "Haggai 0. 8.8.3... o_s.w=.HJmm. 5.3 5.823”. 83:98.0... 5% 038,—. .8. v a ._ .8. v a . 3 u .31.... n .2 .R u 2 .. 6.. u m. 5.. u .x .2 u 2. a... 8... 3... .w... 3.8.3.3. x 8...... 3... 8... 8... 8... 888....-.3. .3 8... 2.. m... 8...... ...o S... E... .2. 8.80 .....Eo.=. and .cd 8.. Led w=.w:o_om-‘.mm. X .23.... 8...- 8... 8.. .5: 3.8.333. .8 E... .5... .8... 8...... ...o 8... b... b... 8.8... Massim— ._ Ny... .2 .. 2...... 805.5 35.8.3.3. o. $50.3... mince—omimm. 5.3 8.32»...— ...o.._o......o.: .55. 03.... 47 48 interaction was significant for externals rather than internals (AR2 = .05, AF = 8.65, p < .01). Further, social support in this interaction acts as a negative buffer (see Figure 3). That is, the stress to negative adjustment relation is higher for Chinese externals who received more social support than for those who lack such support. Although Chinese revealed no significant buffering effects for the ISEL-Composite or ISEL- subscales, significant main effects of ISEL-Appraisal (AR2 = .04, AF = 6.29, p < .02) and ISEL-Tangible (AR2 = .024. AF = 3.96, p < .05) were found for Chinese externals. Further, although the HASSLE-INTENSITY x ISEL-Composite term was not significant for Chinese externals, the graph of regression lines (see Figure 4) indicates a buffering pattern; individuals with high levels of perceived support (ISEL- Composite) had few or no symptoms. 10- —-- HI SUPPORT (+1 so) 9 ' —l— MED SUPPORT (mean) ->K- LO SUPPORT (.1 sd) 8 .- DIG) 01 l 1r 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 HASSLES Score Figure 3. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISSB: Chinese Exnemals 49 — c—L OD-FUIQVCDCDO - + HI SUPPORT (+1 sd) .. —:— MED SUPPORT (mean) _ -7‘K— LO SUPPORT (-1 sd) — OIQ N O 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 O HASSLES Score Figure 4. Regression of GHQ on HASSLES for 3 values of ISEL: Chinese Externals 50 DISCUSSION The present study had two major purposes: (1) to examine cultural variations in the structure of social networks and the perceived availability and actual receipt of _functional support; and (2) to compare the support utilization process (i.e., the 1'" presence of stress-buffering effects from social support) across cultures. Su'ucturally, Anglos demonstrated larger and denser networks. The proportion of family members in Chinese and Anglo-American social networks was equivalent, but because Anglos had a greater total number of supporters, the number of family members in their networks was greater than that for Chinese. These results may be an artifact of the present research samples. Anglo students, who were younger then their Chinese counterparts (m = 19 yrs., g = 21 yrs.; respectively) may report larger family networks and more support received from this source due to developmental issues such as "attachment." Extensive research indicates that adolescents rely heavily on parents for assistance (see Hill & Holmbeck, 1986 for a review of the literature). Functional support comparisons showed that, Anglos, relative to Chinese, reported 51 52 greater levels of received support (IS SB) as anticipated, but unexpectedly higher levels of perceived support (ISEL). Correlational data for the Chinese sample corroborates the generalist-specialist theory (Bogat et al., 1985) in that the number of family members was positively related to the amount of received support and perceived appraisal support. Therefore, the smaller number of family members in Chinese networks may help explain their lower levels of perceived support. In addition to differences in network structures, as well as perceived availability and actual receipt of functional support, the present study found differences in process of support utilization across cultures. Anglos generally indicated greater stress-buffering effects from social support than Chinese. While locus of control appears to mediate the predictive power of the stress by social support interaction for adjustment in b0th cultures. the specific relationship between locus of control and these three variables--stress, social support, and adjustment-~secms to vary across cultures. Anglo results were in accordance with extant research that evidences stress-buffering effects for only internal locus of control individuals (e.g., Cummins, 1988; Lefcourt et al., 1984; Sandler & Lakey, 1982). Interestingly though, neither received nor perceived social support were directly or indirectly beneficial for Chinese internals. These findings not only suggest that the influence of social support is differential across cultures, it also appears that the influence of locus of control is not absolute. Instead, locus of control may interact with cultural norms and values to affect behavior and experience differentially across ethnic groups. Being internal, 53 versus external, may have the universal effect of amplifying one’s ability or tendency to respond actively and adaptively; but what is considered active or adaptive may be relative to one’s own cultural climate. Among Anglo-Americans, the more adaptive response to stress may involve actively and effectually accruing and utilizing support resources. The Chinese cultural ideal, which expects self-discipline from those with high education and high social status, may prescribe more self-directed coping strategies (e.g., controlling oneself, modifying personal expectations), rather than help-seeking. For instance, Wu (1982) suggested that the dilution of stress for Chinese is associated with the ability to "correct the mind and train the temperament" (p. 297). A study employing Hong Kong university students demonstrated that in situations of mild distress, the most prevalent strategies of active coping involved analyzing the problem, resetting goals, and working harder (Cheung, Lee & Chan, 1983). Psychological endurance (i.e., telling oneself to be calm, to accept or forget the problem, and to control one’s thoughts) was also a frequent coping strategy for individuals undergoing mild stress. When problems became more severe, students consulted with medical professionals, rather than family or friends. This behavior seems likely to be related to the documented tendency for Asians to somaticize emotional distress or the need to "save face" and protect the reputation of one’s family by concealing severe problems from network members. For Chinese extemals, received support was not only unrelated to better adjustment, but also had a negative stress-buffering effect. That is, the stress-illness 54 relationship was strongest for Chinese externals who received the most social support. The Chinese tendency to evince emotional restraint (Argyle, Henderson, Bond, Iizuka, & Contarello, 1984; cited by Bond, 1986) may make stressful/difficult times less readily detectable to others so that help is seldom initiated without a direct request from an individual. Therefore, in order to receive help Chinese may be forced to request it explicitly; this may be a stressful undertaking in itself. Among Chinese, requesting assistance from others may signify exposing one’s vulnerability and/or incompetence, and risking rejection. These potential consequences are antithetical to seeking "face," the Chinese endeavor to enhance one’s social status by presenting oneself as better adjusted, more competent, and possessing better social ties than may actually be the case (Bond, 1986). The tendency for Chinese to act in accordance with external expectations or social norms, rather than with internal wishes may further explain their reluctance to request help in times of need (e.g., Yang, 1981). Based on these theories, the cost incurred in soliciting support may well outweigh the benefits gained from its receipt. Perceived appraisal and perceived tangible support, however, had a beneficial effect for Chinese extemals, irrespective of stress level. Perceived support also exhibited a buffering pattern for this group. Hence, perceiving that these types of support are available if needed may be more instrumental in alleviating the effects of stress on adjustment than actually receiving support for Chinese. The findings discussed thus far corroborate Cohen and Wills’ contention that the detection of stress-buffering effects depends on the type of social support measure 55 employed. Specifically, the ISEL and ISSB were differentially successful in predicting adjustment and yielding stress-buffering effects. Measure type alone, however, does not account for the results of this study. A more precise social support theory would incorporate the instrument’s interaction with the culture to which it is applied. Specifically, a nonstatistical comparison of Anglo-American beta weights for received versus perceived support suggests that received support (ISSB) is more stress- buffering, whereas perceived support (ISEL-Composite) is more directly beneficial (main effect). In contrast, the differential effects of received versus perceived support were even greater for Chinese; received support is not only unhelpful during times of high stress, but is related to greater negative adjustment under these circumstances. Further, perceived availability of support, relative to actual receipt of support, is of greater direct benefit (main effect) for the Chinese. Hence, despite the use of what Cohen and Wills (1985) deem "appropriate" measures to detect buffering effects, results of this study corroborate previous research that suggests a lack of bufi‘ering effects among Chinese (e.g., Lin et al., 1979). The present findings suggest a new, more complicated model for social support utilization than those proposed by past studies (see Figure 5). While former studies have acknowledged the separate influences of locus of control, culture, and measure type, the present study demonstrates the interaction of these variables to determine the presence of stress-buffering effects. Specifically, culture moderates the influence of locus of control on social support process differentially across measure types. ._.Zm_>_._.w3_..n_< HmOnEDm ._<_OOm /. .d MIPSDO \ Figure 5. The New Social Support Model 56 57 Limitations of the Study The present research represents a first attempt to integrate former research on social support models with measurement/methodology issues, person variables, and ethnic or cultural influences. Future researchers should be advised of limitations specific to this study, as well as those more generally inherent in cross-cultural research. Sample Biases The Chinese sample may lack generalizability for two reasons. First, university students make up a considerably smaller proportion of the population in China than in America Entry into virtually any university in China is uncommon and limited to only the most competent, most motivated, and perhaps the most independent individuals who defy the standards of the status quo (as exemplified by historical uprisings led by students in Beijing). Further, as a result of the scarcity of universities in China, students are typically forced to relocate to cities that are geographically removed from their hometowns. Family and other established support resources may be less accessible to these individuals, than to American students who have relocated, due to the lack of transportation and communication resources. Finally, the present sample involves Chinese universities that are located in two of the most urbanized and Westemized regions of the country. Hence, students in both of these locations, relative to the whole of China, may be more apt to resemble American values and qualities in their interpersonal relationships. 58 Mggmment Biases Another methodological difficulty present in cross-cultural research is that culture-based explanations can often be rivaled by those implicating measurement artifacts. In this study, each structural and functional support finding may not only be a result of cultural influences but also measurement biases. First, responses on the structural assessment items may be unreliable for the Chinese sample due to the inherent complexity of the instructions and because these types of measures are simply less familiar to foreign populations. This hypothesis is supported by the low completion rate of the structural measures among Chinese. Second, substantially smaller reliability coefficients for the ISEL subscales — among Chinese may also suggest an artifactual explanation for lower levels of perceived support. Lower ISEL scores for Chinese may be due, in part, to the presence of many items in this scale that are less germane to Chinese culture. Because a number of items from the ISEL pertain to specific displays of support (e.g., "If I wanted a date for a party next weekend, I know someone at school or in town who would fix me up"), this scale is highly susceptible to cultural bias. For example, due to cultural mores that limit "casual dating" in China, it is less conventional for Chinese than for Americans to "fix a fiiend up for a weekend date. " Other examples of items less relevant to Chinese may be "I don’t know someone who would loan me several hundred dollars to pay a doctor bill or dental bill" or "Even if I needed it my family would (or‘could) not give me money for tuition or books," both because Chinese typically have less financial resources and because the Chinese government 59 generally subsidizes medical care and tuition. Third, the failure to find positive, buffering effects of received support among Chinese may similarly result from a measurement artifact suggested by previous research (Cohen & Hoberman, 1983; Sandler & Lakey, 1982; both cited by Cohen & Wills, 1985). Specifically, the ISSB is likely to reflect increased use of support due to psychological distress for certain populations. This theory is corroborated by both the positive correlation between the ISSB and stressful life events (especially significant for Chinese) and the presence of crossed interactions in each case where the 1883’s buffering effects are detected (see Figures 2 and 3). For Anglos, the ISSB was associated with lower levels of symptomatology for high-stress individuals and higher levels of symptomatology for low-stress individuals. A negative relation between the IS SB and symptomatology existed for Chinese individuals under low stress, but not for those under high stress. Hence, because the ISSB confounds the availability of support with the need for and use of support, it may not accurately reflect the positive consequences of using available support among Chinese. Finally, as previously mentioned, measures initially developed for Western populations are potentially biased and insensitive to certain social support qualities specific to Chinese. For example, government subsidies for medical care, tuition, room and board, and job placement/security can be considered types of support available to Chinese students that are not included in support measures developed for Western populations. The present study corroborates previous research suggesting that different attributes of social support may be beneficial in different cultures. In two 60 such studies (Cauce, Felner, & Primavera, 1982; Jung & Khalsa, 1989), lower levels of depression were related to perceived family support for African-Americans, and perceived friend support for Anglo-Americans. Unfortunately, the social support measures employed in the present study fail to discriminate between perceived family support and perceived friend support. Based on the similarities described earlier between Asian and African-American social network characteristics and use of social support, it is likely that Chinese, in comparison with Anglos, would report greater perceived family support and stress-buffering effects from this type of support. Future research may begin investigating this hypothesis by including Procidano and Heller’s (1979) perceived support of family and friends scales. Future Directions The present study provides clear evidence of the need to implicate cultural factors in future examinations of the provision, perception, receipt, and utilization of social support. Not only would such a pursuit generate a broader information base regarding social support in other countries, but it would also enhance conceptualizations regarding social support across subcultures within America. Therefore, the present research should be replicated with various ethnic populations. A subsequent step may involve expanding two-culture comparisons to multicultural comparisons for the purposes of minimizing plausible rival hypotheses. For instance, in order to isolate ethnic influences from political, economic, and social influences, futtue investigations should obtain a more representative sample of mainland Chinese with which other segments of the Chinese population at large (e. g., 61 Chinese in Hong Kong, Taiwan, and Singapore) can be compared. A more stringent test of the present hypotheses would be affirmed if findings of this study generalized in varying degrees, across the host of cultures outside of China that describe themselves as "Chinese." To assure cross-cultural validity of such studies, scale development is recommended In particular, social support measures should be tailored for cross- cultural relevance by the addition of items assessing Chinese displays of social support and the exclusion of irrelevant items. In sum, findings of the present study demonstrate the need to recognize the unique (emic) features, as well as the comparable (etic) features of social relations in various cultures. What is needed is a theory of cultural variation that can be incorporated into existing theories regarding the relationship between stress, social support, adjustment, and relevant person variables. The resulting theory would place each ethnic group on some map of cultural dimensions, with groups that share social, economic, or political qualities in closer proximity. In this way, we could begin predicting how individuals fi'om cultures differing in known ways should differ from one another in relevant behaviors and responses. Information about an individual’s culture could then function like information about his or her personality (e. g., locus of control, social competency) in generating predictions fiom theories of social support. Appendix A Directions: In this section you will find a series of attitude statements. Each represents a commonly held opinion. There are no right or wrong answers. You will probably agree with some items and disagree with others. We are interested in the extent to which you agree or disagree with such matters of opinion. Read each statement carefully. Then circle the number that best indicates the extent to which you agree or disagree. First opinions are usually best. Give your opinion on every Statement. X8. 10. ll. 12. 13. Strongly Agree = l Disagree Somewhat = 2 Slightly Disagree = 3 Slightly Agree = 4 Agree Somewhat = 5 Strongly Agree = 6 Whether or not I get to be a leader depends mostly on my ability. To a great extent my life is controlled by accidental happenings. I feel like what happens in my life is mostly determined by powerful people. Whether or not I get into a car accident depends mostly on how good a driver I am. When I make plans, I am almost certain to make them work. Often there is no chance of protecting my personal interests fiom bad luck happenings. When I get what I want, it’s usually because I’m lucky. Although I might have good ability, I will not be given leadership responsibility without appealing to those in positions of power. How many friends I have depends on how nice a person I am. I have often found that what is going to happen will happen. My life is chiefly controlled by powerful others. Whether or not I get into a car accident is mostly a matter of luck. People like myself have very little chance of protecting our personal interests when they conflict with those of strong pressure groups. 62 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. It’s not always wise for me to plan too far ahead because many things turn out to be a matter of good or bad fortune. Getting what I want requires pleasing those people above me. Whether or not I get to be a leader depends on whether I’m lucky enough to be in the right place at the right time. If important people were to decide they didn’t like me, I probably wouldn’t make many friends. I can pretty much determine what will happen in my life. I am usually able to prOtect my personal interests. Whether or not I get into a car accident depends mostly on the orher driver. When I get what I want, it’s usually because I worded hard for it. In order to have my plans work, I make sure that they fit in with the desires of people who have power over me. My life is determined by my own actions. It’s chiefly a matter of fate whether or n0t I have a few friends or many friends. 63 Appendix B INSTRUCTIONS We are interested in learning about some of the ways that you P people have helped you or tried to make life more pleasant for over the past four weeks. Below you will find a list of activitit other people might have done for you, to you, or with you 5 weeks. Please read each item carefully and indicate how r .: activities happened to you during the past four weeks. Use the following scale to make your ratings: A. Not at all 8. Once or twice C. About once a week 0. Several times a week ti. About every day Make all of your ratings on the answer s' «as been provided. If, for example, the item: 45. Gave you a ride to the docto' happened once or twice during .our weeks. you would make your rating like this: A B C l) E 45.13 I D D r Please read each ’ .t'ully and select the rating that you think is the most accurr During the past four weeks, how oflen did other peeple do t' activities for you. to you, or- with you: set-“t“.- l5. l6. IT. IS. I9. 20. 2|. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 3f 37. 38. 39. «l. PPFP’9‘1“ Looked alter a family member when you were away. Was right there with you (physically) in a stressful situ' Provided you with a place where you could get away .e. Watched after your possessions when you were away ants. home, apartment, etc.). Told you what she/he did in a situation that was .v yours. Did some activity with you to help you get your ' if things. Talked with you about some interests of yours Let you know that you did something well. Went with you to someone who could take I Told you that you are OK just the way yo: Told you that she/he would keep the th' you talk about private ——just between the two of you. . Assisted you in setting a goal for your l3. l4. Made it clear what was expected of r Expressed esteem or respect for a c .y or personal quality of yours. Gave you some information on i- something. Suggested some action that you ake. Gave you over $25. Comfortcd you-by showing r physical affection. Gave you some informatior ou understand a situation you were in. Provided you with some .atlon. Checked back with you you followed the advice you were given. Gave you under 523. Helped you unders you didn’t do something well. Listened to you tr your private feelings. Loaned or gave .nething (a physical object other than money) that yo Agreed that v wanted to do was right. Said things ' .' your situation clearer and easier to under- stand. Told you ' ne felt in a situation that was similar to yours. Let you ' t helshe will always be around if you need assist- ance. Eaprr rest and concern in your well-being. Told . she/he feels very close to you. To' to you should see for assistance. T that to expect in a situation that was about to happen. ' ou over $25. you how to do something. you feedback on how you were doing without saying it was go. or bad. loked and kidded to try to cheer you up. Provided you with a place to stay. Pitched in to help you do something that needed to get done. Loaned you under $25. 65 Appendix C Directions: This scale is made up of a list of statements each of which may or may not be true about you. For each statement we would like you to enter T if the statement is true about you or P if the statement is not true about you. You may find that many of the statements are neither clearly true nor clearly false. In these cases, try to decide quickly whether probably TRUE or probably FALSE is most descriptive of you. Although some questions will be difficult to answer, it's important that you pick one alternative or the other. Remember to circle only one of the alternatives for each statement. Please read each item quickly but carefully ' “ore responding. Remember that this is not a test and there are r t or wrong answers. 70) I know someone who would loan me $50 to gr for the weekend. 71) I don't know anyone at school or in towr akes my problems clearer and easier to understand. 72) Most of my friends have more control .nat happens to them than I. 73) I will have a better future than me er people will. 74) I hang out in a friend's room or ant quite a lot. 75) I don't talk to a member of my ’ at least once a week. 76) I can get a date who I enjoy r .9 time with whenever I want. 77) If I decided at dinner time a a study break this evening and go to a movie, I could easily someone to go with me. 78) Most of my friends don't well as I do in school; 79) Most of my friends are atisfisd or happier with themselves than I am. 80) I know someone at sr .r in town who would bring my meals to my room or apartment if I ick. 81) If I needed it, ' ily would provide me with an allowance and spending money 82) Most people v' .w me well think highly of me. 83) I don't kno' J. at school or in town who would loan me their bicycle o' ror a couple of hours. 84) If I war date for a party next weekend, I know someone at school or in no would fix me up. 85) Peop' ; out in my room or home during the day or in the evening. 86) I 8 .now anyone at school or in town who would get assignments f from my teachers if I was sick. 87) La. 1, when I've been troubled, I keep things to myself. 88) I am not a member of any social groups (e.g., clubs, teams). 89) I don't often get invited to do things with other people. 90) I know someone who I see or talk to often with whom I would feel perfectly comfortable talking about any problems I might have meeting people. 91) Most people are more attractive than I am. 66 92) I know someone who I see or talk to often with whom I would feel perfectly comfortable talking about any problems I might have making friends. 93) I don't know anyone at school or in town who would help me study for an exam by spending several hours reading me questions. 94) I know someone who I see or talk to often with wh' I would feel perfectly comfortable talking about any difficul ith my social life. 95) Lately, I often feel lonely, like I don't hav ne to reach out to. 96) I know someone who I see or talk to often w5 :m I would feel perfectly comfortable talking about any prr I might have getting along with my parents. 97) There isn't anyone at school or in town lhom I would feel perfectly comfortable talking about my age of loneliness and depression. 98) Most of my friends think that I'm s' 99) I don't usually spend two evenings .e weekend doing something with others. 100) I know someone who I see or ta' often with whom I would feel perfectly comfortable taking problems I might have budgeting my time between school and my - life. 101) Most of my friends are mor .lar than I am. 102) Most of my friends have ' Justed to college as easily as I have. 103) Most people think I hav .od sense of humor. 104) I don't have friends 3 gel or in town who would comfort me by showing some physica' .ction. 105) I don't feel frien? ih any teaching assistants, professors, campus or student offici' 106) There are people ;hool or in town who I regularly run with, exercise with, ay sports with. 107) Most of my fr are more interesting than I am. 108) I know somer : I see or talk to often with whom I would feel perfectly table talking about any problems I might have adjusting allege life. 109) I belong to a group at school or in town that meets regularly or does things together regularly. 110) Even if I needed it my family would (or could) not give me money for tuition or books. 111) I don't know anyone who would loan me several hundred dollars to pay a doctor bill or dental bill. *'*YOU SHOULD NOW BE UP TO LINE 112 ON YOUR ANSWER SHEET*** 67 Appendix D In the left hand column, list the initials of up to 20 people whom you consider important in your life ml. with whom on are likely to interact at least once during any two week griod. Next, mdicate whe er each person is male or female. Then, circ e 0 one number that best indicates his/her relauonship to you (e.g. "friend," "immediate Tia—1:377 Remember, you can list Less than 20 people. ' nele (I) or Female (7)? ’ Initials i) ' r 1 2 . a 4 3 9 7 9 9 2) __ 1 r 1 2 a 4 s 9" ‘7 9 9 z) , r 1 z a 4 s 4 7 9.. 9 4) __ a r 1 z s 4 s 9 7 9 9 j s) _____ l r 1 z 3 4 5 6 7 I 9 , 5) l 7 1 2 3 4 3 G 7 O O 7) ‘ F 1 2 3 6 5 G 7 I 9 3) ‘ I 1 2 3 t 3 5 7 I 9 9) ' ‘ ’ 1 g 3 6 3 ‘ 7 O C to) ._ x r t 2 3 ‘ 3 ‘ 7 ’ ’ 11). .__ s r 1 2 3 9 5 ‘ ’ ' ’ .2, I Y 1 2 3 s 5 6 7 O 9 :3) __ s r 1 z 3 4 s -s z 9 9 :4) __ x r 1 2. a 4 s 4 7 9 9 is) __ x r i 2 3 9 3 ‘ ’ ' . ’ :0 __ s r t z 3 4 s 6 7 ‘ ’ -7) __ s r 1 2 3 4 5 ‘ 7 ' ’ a) __ s r 2 a 4 s 4 7 I 9 v) __ s r 1 z a 4 s 9 7 9 9 2 __ I! r 1 a a 4 s 4 7 9 9 68 Please think about your interactions with the people that you listed in the left hand column. Of the cate ories described below, indicate which kind(s) of support each person can give you. (CIR LE ALL THAT APPLY): A. B. EMOTIONAL SUPPORT includes: listening to your troubles, being understanding, comforting, and sympathetic. ADVICE'& INFORMATION includes: sharing what they would do in a situation like yours, hel ing you get the info you need to help yourself helping you think through a prob em. PRACTICAL ASSISTANCE includes: helping by doing things for you (for example, running errands for you, taking notes for you, taking notes for you in class), lending you money. COMPANION SHIP includes: spending time with you in leisure and recreational activities (for example, going with you to dinner or to the movies. k M 1) 2) 3) 6) 5) 000000 6) 7) 8) 9) 10) 13) 14) 2.5:). 16) 17) 18) D D D D D D D D D D D D D D D D D D 19) D D A D A D A D A D A D A D A D A D A D A D 11) A D A D A D A D A D A D A D A 3 A D A 3 00000000000000 20) 69 Appendix E Label the columns and rows with the initials of up to ten people (from your list on the previous page) whom you feel closest to. Then, indicate who of these people know each other by placing an 'X' in the square where the two names intersect. (Only fill in the unshaded squares.) For exam le° If 31. knows SR 5L SRHM BC place anp'llt:1 in Ehe square ' 3'1, 7 X where JL an SR ntersect. SK (fig . ./ Tfigéz ' BC (MC/A/ 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 \F\\\‘ \s \ \ . i . .....\\§- \< \\ 10 Appendix F Directions: Hassles ere irritants that can rang; from minor annoyances to fairly major pressures, problems, or difficulties. They can occur few or many times. Listed below are a number of ways in which a person can feel hassled. 0n the answer sheet, indicate (by entering a i, 2. or 1) how SEVERE each hassle has been for you in the past month. For as hassle that did lg: occur in the last month, enter a five (5). £2 1. S! 3 severe HASSLZS 2. v .aly severe 1 . .ely severe s. at occur 112) hisplacing or losing things 111) Troublesome neighbors 114) Social obligations 115) Inconsiderate smokers 116) Troubling thoughts about your 117) Thoughts about death . 116) Health of a family member 119) not enough money for cloth' 120) Concerns about owing mone 121) Concerns about money for ancies 122) Someone owes you money 121) Financial responsibil' someone who doesn't live with you 124) Smoking too much 125) Use of alcohol 126) Too many responait s 127) Concerned about t ning of life 128) Trouble relaxinr 129) Trouble making .ons 110) Prouiess gett sng with roommates 111) Don't like r area of study 112) Don't like ates 111) Not enouq' for basic necessities l14) Not enou .y for food 115) Too men .ruptions 116) Unexpe ampany s 111) Too much as on hands 116) Having to wait 119) Concerns about accidents 71 140) 141) 142) 143) 144) 14:) 144) 147) 144) 149) 150) 151) 1:3) 153) 154) 155) 154) 157) 15a) 159) 140) 141) 142) 14:) 144) 145) 1“) ' 167 171) 172) 17:) 174) 175) 174) 177) Being lonely Fear of confrontation Financial security Silly practical mistakes Inability to express yourself Physical illness side effects of medication Concerns about medical treatment Physical appearance rear of rejection Concerns about health in general hot seeing enough people friends or relatives too far Heating time Being exploited Not getting enough slec Problems with aging r Problems with your cant other Overloaded with ' asponsibilities Too many thingr Unchallenginr Concerns a' .ting high standards Academic .es Gossip Cone sut weight no’ 1 time to do the things you need to do agh personal energy :na about inner conflicts l conflicted over what to do agrets over past decisions The weather Nightmares Concerns about getting ahead Difficulties with friends Not enough time for family not enough money for transportation Not enough money for entertainment and recreation Concerns about news events Appendix G Please read this carefully: Us would like to know if you have had any medical complaints. and how your health has been in general, over tbs past few weeks. Please answer ALL the questions on the following pages simply by circling the answer which you think most nearly applies to you. leaemher that we want to know about present and recent complaints. not those that you had in the past. It is impor‘ hat you try to answer ALL the questions. Thank you vet \ for your cooperation. RAVI YOU RWY: ' to get back to sleep? 73 t. — been feeling pale. tter Same Worse Much worse well and in (and \ usual as usual than usual than usual health? 2. — been feeling in need sli No more Rather more Much more of “u "“0““ than usual than usual than usual 3. — aees leelia; Nt. No more Rather more Much more and and than usual than usual than usual exhausted? 4. —jelt that you are ill? Not at . 'o more Rather more Much more n usual than usual than usual 5. — been telling any Not at all tore Rather more Much more pains in your head? susl than usual than usual 6. — been getting a feel- Not at all . ~ Rather more Much more ' in; a] tightness or tl. ' than usual than usual pnuncebyyuw ham” 7. - been able to can- Better Same ”.ess Much less emtrate on what- than ' as usuat \n usual than usual nurymuVadhth uuufl I. -— been afraid that you Not at all No more 4' more Much more soars (sin; to col- than usual ‘ual than usual knusnhspuufi: place) 9. — been ltam'rq ltat or Not at all No more Rat. -. Much more caltl spells) than usual than than usual to. — been permit-in; Not at all No more Rather Much more (meeting) a lot? than usual than ust. ‘tan usual I t. - found yourself toab- Not at all No more Rather the Much more in: early and unable than usual than usual than usual IIAVI YOU “huh" i'LY: t2. — been (cunt; up feel- Not at all No more in: your sleep hasn't than usual refreshed you? 13. — been feeling too tired Not at all No more and exhausted even than usual to eat? 14. — lost much sleep Not at all No more oorr tuorrv’ than usual 15. — been fee tally Better Same alert one than usual as usual awake? t6. — been feeling , Better Same energy? than usual as usual 17. — had dalfiiculty in hit at all No more (sun): of to slee, than usual 18. — had dtfiatlty in at all No more stayt'm asleep arses than usual you are 0.0? 19. -— been having 1* ‘ No more frightening or than usual unpleasant dreams? 20. — been having restless, Not a. No more disturbed nights? ‘tan usual 2!. — been managing to More so no keep yourself busy than usual mal and occupied? 22. -— been taking longer Quicker over the thugs you than usual a do? . a3. — tended to late Net at all No . interest in your than \ ordinary activities? 24. — been losing interest Not at all No snore in your personal than usual appearance? as. - been taking less More About trouble nn'tlt your trouble same clothes? than usual as usual 26. — been getting out of More Same the house as mud! than usual as usual as usual? 27. - been managing or Better About well at most people than most the same would in your shoes? 23. -—[elt on the toltole Better About you were doing than usual the same things well? 74 Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Less alert than usual Less energy than usual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Rather more titan usual Rather more than usual Rather less than usual Longer than usual Rather more than usual ‘her more usual \uble Les. than Rather less well Less well than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much more titan usual Much less alert Much less energetic Much more than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much less than usual Much longer than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much less trouble Much less than usual Much less well Much less well 29. 30. 3s. 32. 33- 35- 36. 37- 38. 39- so. as. 42. 43. 44- - been late getting to soorh, or gettitc started on your housesoorh? — been satisfied with the my you've carried out your task? — been able to feel unsrrrstls and qa'ec- tion for those near to you? -— been finding it easy to get on with other people? — spent much time chatting with push? . — hept feeling afraid to say anything to people in case you made a fool of yourself? - felt that you are play- ing a useful part in shims? —- felt capable of snahing decisions about things? — felt you're just not able to snobs a start on anythisu? — felt yourself dread- ing everything that you have to do? — felt constantly under strain? — felt you couldn't overcome your difiiculsies? — been fittdit struggle - been I your to-day a. — been tahing thiscs hard? — been gettisq edgy and bad-tempered? my Jay- Jities? us? Not at all More satisfied Better than usual Better than usual More time than usual Not at all More so than usual More so than usual Nos at al' 140' at at all Not at all More so than usual Not at all Not at all No later than usual About same as usual About sasne as usual About same as usual About same as usual No snore than usual Same as us' '0 more .an usual No more than usual No more than usual No more than usual No more than usual Same as usual No more than usual No more than usual 75 Rather later than usual Less satisfied than usual Less well than usual Less well than usual Less thanv Rr re .ess useful than usual Lessso thanusual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Less so than usual Rather more than usual Rather more than usual Much later than usual Much less satisfied Much less well .688 Auch less than usual Much more than usual Much less useful Muchless capable Much more than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual Much less than usual Much more than usual Much more than usual IMVI YOU “GUN l'LY: 45. -— been getting seared Not at all No more Rather more Much nture or pasu'chy for no than usual than usual than usual good reason? 46. — been able to face up More so Same Less able Much less to your problesns? than usual as usual than usual able 47. -— found everythisc Not at all No more Rather more Much more getting on top of you? than usual than usual than usual 48. — had the feeling that Not at all No more Rather more Ms ch more people were looking than usual than usual t' ‘ual at you? 49. — been feeling unhappy Not at all No more Rather snort . snare and depressed? than usual than usua’ .s usual 50. — been losing confi- Not at all No more Rather Auch more dence in yourself? than usual than . than usual 5:. - been thinhing of Not at all No more Rs' 3 Much more yourself as a than usual t’ 4 than usual worthless person? 52. — felt that life is Not at all No more .-r snore Much more entirely hopeless? than usu .s usual than usual 53. — been feeling hopeful More so About .css so Much less about your own than usual sanv than usual hopeful future? as 54. — been feeling reason- More so Less so Much less ably happy. all than usual than usual than usual things considered? ual 55. -— been feeling nervous Not at .s more Rather more Much more and strum-up all .han usual than usual than usual the time? 56. — felt that life isn't ’ . No more Rather snore Much more worth living? than usual than usual than usual 57. — “.9”;- “r n, .u'tely I don't llas crossed Definitely peeesasss e; s . think so my mind have you as gbc or tour 1 s I e r 58. — found at I' Not at all No more Rather more Much more couldn't .ng than usual than usual than usual becaus rues were 59. — foun. self wish- Not at all No more Rather more Much more ing you uere dead than usual than usual than usual and away front is all? 60. -— found that the idea Definitely 1 don't Has crossed Definitely of taking your own not think so my mind has life hept causing into your mind? 76 Appendix H MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY PARTICIPATION CONSENT FORM This project regards the social networks, health, and perspectives of college students. You will receive 3 extra credit points for completing a questionnaire that will take between 60-90 minutes to complete. Your answers will be totally anonymous and confidential. PLEASE DO NOT PUT YOUR NAME ON THE QUESTIONNAIRE. We do ask, however, that you answer the questions honestly and thoughtfully. Thank you for your time and consideration. 1) The study has been explained to me. I understand the explanation that has been given and what my participation involves. 2) I understand that my participation is entirely voluntary and that I am free to stop participating in the testing session at any time without penalty. 3) I understand that my responses on the questionnaire will be strictly confidential and anonymous. 4) I understand that my participation in this research will not quarantee any direct benefits to me. 5) I am at least 18 yrs. of age or have signed parental consent. 6) I understand that I can discuss any feelings about my participation in this study with Belle Liang (52 Baker Hall, phone #: 355-7440). 7) I understand that I will no receive the 3 credits unless and until I participate in my scheduled testing session. 8) My completion of the following questionnaire is proof of my consent to partcipate in the research project. 77 REFERENCES Aldwin, C., & Greenberger, E. (1987). Cultural differences in predictors of depression. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 789-813. Andrews. 6., Tennant, C., Hewson, D. M., & Vaillant, G. E. (1978). Life event stress, social support, coping style, and risk of psychological impairment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 166, 307-316. Argyle, M., Henderson, M., Bond, M. H., Iizuka, Y., & Contarello, A. (1984). Cross- cultural variations in relationship rules. Unpublished manuscript, Oxford University. Ball, R. E., Warheit, G. J., Vandiver, J. S., & Holzer, C. E. (1980). Friendship networks: More supportive of low-income Black women. Ethnicity, 7, 70-77. Ball, R. E., Warheit, G. 1., Vandiver, J. S., Holzer, C. E. (1979). Kin ties of low-income blacks and whites. Ethnicity, 6, 184—196. 78 79 Barrera, M. (1981). Social support in the adjustment of pregnant adolescents: Assessment issues. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed), Social networks and social support (pp. 69-96). Beverly Hills: Sage. Barrera, M., Sandler, I., & Ramsey, T. B. (1981). Stress and the environment. Journal of Social Issues, 37, 435-447. Bengtson, V. L., & Smith, D. H. (1968). Social modernity and attitudes toward aging: A cross-cultural survey. Gerontologist, 8, 26. Bogat, G. A., Caldwell, R. A., Rogosch, F. A., & Kriegler, J. A. (1985). Differentiating specialists and generalists within college students’ social support networks. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 14, 23-35. Bond, M. H. (1986). The psychology of the Chinese people. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press. Bond, M. H., & Tomatzky, L. G. (1973). Locus of control in students from Japan and the United States. Psychologia, 16, 209-213. Burks, N. B., & Martin, B. (1985). Everyday problems and life change events: Ongoing versus acute sources of stress. Journal of Human Stress, 11, 27-35. Caplan, G. (1974). Support systems and community mental health. New York: Behavioral Publications. Cauce, A. M., Felner, R. D., & Primavera, J. (1982). Social support in high-risk adolescents: Structural components and adaptive impact. American Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 417-428. 80 Chan, D. W. (1985). The Chinese version of the General Health Questionnaire: Does language make a difference? Psychological Medicine, 15, 147-155. Chan, D. W. (1986). Psychosocial mediators in the life event-illness relationship. International Journal of Psychosomatics, 33, 3-10. Chan, D. W. (1989). Dimensionality and adjustment correlates of locus of control among Hong Kong Chinese. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 145-160. Chan, D. W., & Chan, T. S. (1983). Reliability, validity and the structure of the General Health Questionnaire in a Chinese context. Psychological Medicine, 13, 363-371. Chan, D. W., & Chan-Ho, M. W., Chan, T. (1984). Life event scaling: The Chinese experience. Social Science Medicine, 3, 3-7. Chang, B. L., Chang, A. L., & Shen, Y. (1984). Attitudes toward aging in the United States and Taiwan. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 15 , 109-129. Cheung, F. M. (1982). Psychological symptoms among Chinese in urban Hong Kong. Social Science and Medicine, 16, 1339-44. Cheung, F. M. (1984). Preferences in help-seeking among Chinese students. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 8, 371-380. Cheung, F. M., Lee, S. Y. , & Chan, Y. Y. (1983). Variations in problem conceptualizations and intended solutions among Hong Kong students. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 7, 263-278. Cochran, M., & Brassard, J. (1979). Child development and personal social networks. Child Development, 50, 601-616. 81 Cohen, R, Struening, E. L., Muhlin, G. L., Genevie, L. E., Kaplan, S. R., & Peck, H. B. (1982). Community stressors, mediating conditions, and well being in urban neighborhoods. Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 377-391. Cohen, S., & Hoberman, H. (1983). Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 99-125. Cohen, S., & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress and buffering hypothesis: A theoretical analysis. In A. Baum, J. E. Singer, & S. E. Taylor (Eds), Handbook of psychology and health. Vol. 4. (pp. 253-267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohen, 8., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357. Cook, H., & Chi, C. (1984). Cooperative behavior and locus of control among American and Chinese-American boys. Journal of Psychology, 118, 169-177. Cummins, R. C. (1988). Perceptions of social support, receipt of supportive behaviors, locus of control as moderators of the effects of chronic stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 685-700. Delcngis, A., Coyne, 1., Dakof, G., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. (1982). Relationship of daily hassles, uplifts, and major life events to health status. Health Psychology, 1, 119-136. Eaton, W. W. (1978). Life events, social supports, and psychiatric symptoms: A reanalysis of the New Haven data. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 230-234. 82 Fischer, C. S. (1982). The dispersion of kinship ties in modern society: Contemporary data and historical speculation. Journal of Family History, 7, 353-375. Goldberg, D. P. (1972). The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire. Oxford University Press: London. Gore, 8. (1981). Stress-buffering functions of social supports: An appraisal and clarification of research models. In B. S. Dohrenwend & B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds), Stressfid life events and their contexts. New York: Prodist. Graves, T. D., & Graves, N. B. (1985). Stress and health among Polynesian migrants to New Zealand. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 8, 1-19. Hill, J. P., & Holmbeck, G. N. (1986). Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. Annals of Child Development, 2, 145-189. Hirsch, B. J. (1979). Psychological dimensions of social networks: A multimethod analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 8, 159-172. Hirsch, B. J. (1980). Natural support systems and coping with major life changes. American Journal of Community Psychology, 8, 159-172. Hofstede, G. (1983). Dimensions of national cultures in fifty countries and three regions. In J. B. Deregowski, S. Dziurawiec, & R. C. Annis (Eds), Explications in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 335-55). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger. Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218. 83 Hsieh, T. T., Shybut, J ., & Lotsof, E. J. (1969). Internal versus external control and ethnic group membership: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 122-124. Hsu, F. L. K. (1953). Americans and Chinese: Two ways of life. New York: Abelard-Schuman. Hwang, C. H. (1982). Studies in Chinese personality: A critical review. Bulletin of Educational Psychology, 15 , 227-241. Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 1-39. Kleinman, A. M. (1982). Neurasthenia and depression: A study of somatization and culture in China. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 6, 117-90. Kuo, S. Y., & Specs, E. R. (1983). Chinese-American life-styles: A comparative study. Journal of College Student Personnel, 24, 111-117. Lao, R. C. (1977). Levinsons IPC (internal-external control) scale: A comparison of Chinese and American students. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 9, 113-124. Lao, R. C., Chuang, C. J., & Yang, K. S. (1977). Locus of control and Chinese college students. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 8, 299-313. Lefcourt (197 6). Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 84 Lefcourt (1982). Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research, 2nd edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lefcourt, H. M., Miller, R. S., Ware, E. E., & Sherk, D. (1973). Cognitive activity and hypothesis formation during a double entendre word association test as a function of locus of control and field dependence. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 5, 161-173. Lefcourt, H. M., Martin, R. A., & Saleh, W. A. (1984). Locus of control and social support: Interactive moderators of stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 378-389. E Levenson, H. (1974). Activism and powerful others: Distinctions within the concept of internal-external control. Journal of Personality Assessment, 38, 377-383. Levenson, H. (1973). Multidimensional locus of control in psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41, 397-404. Lin, N., Simeone, R. S., Ensel, W. M., & Kuo, W. (1979). Social support, stressful life events, and illness: A model and an empirical test. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 20, 108-119. Mahler, I. (1974). A comparative study of locus of control. Psychologia, I 7, 135-139. McTavish, G. D. (1971). Perceptions of old people: A review of research methodologies and findings. The Gerontologist, 11, 90-101. Mitchell, R. E., & Trickett, E. J. (1980). Task force report: Social networks as mediators of social support. Community Mental Health Journal, 16, 27-44. 85 Nelson, P. C., & Phares, E. J. (1971). Anxiety, discrepancy between need value and expectancy, and internal-external control. Psychological Reports, 28, 663-668. Nuttal, E. V., Chieh, L., & Nuttall, R. L. (1988). Views of the family by Chinese and U.S. children: A comparative study of kinetic family drawings. Journal of School Psychology, 26, 191-194. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347-374. Phares, E. J. (1968). Differential utilization of information as a function of internal-external locus of control. Journal of Personality, 36, 649-662. Phares, E. J. (1976). Locus of control in personality. NJ: Silver Burdett. Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1-24. Raymond, J., Rhoads, D., & Raymond. R. (1981). The relative impact of family and social involvement on Chicano mental health. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1-24. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (Whole No. 609). Sandler, I., & Lakey, B. (1982). Locus of control as a stress moderator: The role of control perceptions and social support. American Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 65-80. 86 Schachter, S. (1959). The psychong of afiiliation: Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Seeman, M. (1963). Alienation and social learning in a reformatory. American Journal of Sociology, 69, 270-284. Seeman, M., & Evans, J. W. (1962). Alienation and learning in a hospital setting. American Sociological Review, 27, 772-783. Stewart, D., & Vaux, A. (1983, May). Ethnicity and social support. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL. Strickland, B. R. (1971). Aspiration responses among Negro and white adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 315-320. Strickland, B. R. (197 8). Internal-external expectancies and health-related behaviors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 1192-1211. Sue, D. W. (1978). Eliminating cultural oppression in counseling: Toward a general theory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25, 419-428. Tseng, M. S. (1972). Attitudes towards the disabled: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Social Psychology, 87, 311-312. Uomoto, J. (1983, April). Social support and the family environment of Asian Americans. Paper presented at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association, San Francisco, California. Vaux, A. (1985). Variations in social support associated with gender, ethnicity, and age. Journal of Social Issues, 41 , 89-110. 87' Vieweg, B. W., & Hedlund, J. L. (1983). The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ): A comprehensive review. Journal of Operational Psychiatry, 14, 74—81. Williams, A. W., Ware, J. E., Jr., & Donald. C. A. (1981). A model of mental health, life events, and social supports applicable to general populations. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 324-336. Walk, 8., & DuCette, J. (1974). Intentional performance and incidental learning as a function of personality and task directions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 90-101. Wu, D. (1982). Psychotherapy and emotion in traditional Chinese medicine. In A. J. Marsella & G. M. White (Eds.), Cultural conceptions of mental health and therapy (pp. 285-301). Dordrecht, Netherlands: D. Reidel. Yang, K. S. (1981). The formation and change of Chinese personality: A cultural-ecological perspective. Acta Psychological Taiwanica, 23, 39-55. Yang, K. S. (1982). Yuan and its functions in modern Chinese life. In Proceedings of the Conference on Traditional Culture and Modern ere (pp. 103-128). Taipei: Committee on the Renaissance of Chinese Culture. Yang, K. S., & Liang, W. H. (1982). Some correlates of achievement motivation among Chinese high school boys. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 15 , 59-67. Yu, L. C., & Harburg, E. (1980). Acculturation and stress among Chinese Americans in a university town. International Journal of Group Tensions, 10, 99-119. ICHIGRN STRTE U HI 111 1111 V I . LIBRRRIES 1 11111111111111 6 1 58 1 312 300 7