lilillllllllllllllllllllllllillllllll 3 1293 00881 0925 This is to certify that the thesis entitled T/te thots of: Drswefany , OISLoA-firm‘fl‘on I and terrific/e Elaborwrc‘on on A't'frrude (Junie presented by Chien—PIV Fa WI! La i has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M0512? degree in O - O Major p ofessor Date f/Br/S‘rl 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution __ , 7, ..fi—# LIBRARY Michigan State 4 Untverslty PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE v a MSU Is An Affirmative ActioNEquel Opportunity Institution cmmpme-pn THE EFFECTS OF DISCREPANCY, DISCONFIRMATION, AND COGNITIVE ELABORATION ON ATTITUDE CHANGE BY Chien-Ping Faith Lai A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Sociology 1991 L. ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF DISCREPANCY, DISCONFIRHATION, AND COGNITIVE ELABORATION ON ATTITUDE CHANGE BY Chien-Ping Faith Lai The effects of discrepancy and disconfirmation on attitude change are reviewed. The role of cognitive elaboration is discussed that source focus which is one kind of cognitive elaboration should induce the effects of disconfirmation on comparative elaboration and issue focus which is the other kind of cognitive elaboration would lead to the effect of comparative evaluation on final position. The results support the two process - two opinion model that discrepancy has a strong direct effect on final position, whereas disconfirmation has a direct effect on comparative evaluation. It is concluded that the more subjects think about the source and engage in the issue, the greater are the effects of disconfirmation on comparative evaluation and comparative evaluation on final position, respectively. Also, some problems of this experiment are discussed. To my parents iii Acknowledgements I wish to express appreciation to my advisor, Prof. Stan Kaplowitz, for his patience, enthusiastic support, and detailed instructions. Also, I am grateful to Prof. Thomas Conner and Cliff Broman for the valuable feedback they have provided. By the same token, I am indebted to a number of good friends. Some have taught me new techniques; some provided service to print questionnaires; further, some have always been there when I need help. Among them are Sping Wang, Jumbo Chang, Shyuee Chen, Jashinta Costa, Show-Hui Hsu, Jackie Larivee, Grace Lin, Bangnan Liu, Peter Liu, Hsin-Ming Liu, Hui-Jane Lu, Diana Pan, Rosa Teng, Elisabeth Waeckerlin, and szy-Meng Yang. Thank all of them and others whose names are not listed here for their friendships which enriched my life at Spartan, Michigan State University. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Vi LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Viii Theoretical Review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 The Experimental Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 Independent Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 Major Dependent Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 Data Transformations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 Manipulation Checks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 Discrepancy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 Disconfirmation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 Cognitive Elaboration . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 Effects on Attitude Change . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 Tests of Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 Discussion and Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 APPENDIX . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 LIST OF TABLES Page Table 1 - Discrepancy and Disconfirmation Levels for the Experimental Design. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 Table 2 - The Definition of Four Forms. . . . . . . . . 14 Table 3 - Skewness for Dependent Variables. . . . . . . 19 Table 4 - Homoscedasticity for Dependent Variables. . . 20 Table 5 - The Means of Subjects' Effort Spent Thinking about Convict X Case (EFFORT) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 Table 6 - The Means of Subjects' Time Spent on the Case of Convict X (TIMSPD) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition23 Table 7 - The Means of Subjects' Total Thoughts on the Whole Questionnaire (TOTTH) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 Table 8 - The Means of Subjects' Source Thoughts by Cognitive Elaboration Condition. . . . . . . . . . 25 Table 9 - The Means of Subjects' Issue Thoughts by Cognitive Elaboration Condition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 Table 10 - The Means of Subjects' Actual Time Spent on Finishing the Whole Questionnaire (TOTATIM) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition. . . . . . . . . . 27 Table 11 - Geometric Mean (95% Confidence Interval) of Subjects' Final Position for Convict X (P1) by Discrepancy and Disconfirmation Level. . . . . . . 28 Table 12 - Adjusted Geometric Mean (95% Confidence Interval) of Comparative Evaluation of Convict X's by Discrepancy and Disconfirmation Level. . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 Table 13 - The Effect of Disconfirmation on Comparative Evaluation 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 30 vi Table 14 - The Effect of Comparative Evaluation on Final Position. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 Table 15 - The Effect of Final Position on Comparative Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 - viii Theoretical Review Festinger and Aronson (1960) suggested that greater the discrepancy between the recipient own opinion and the opinion advocated by the communicator, the greater the dissonance, and hence the greater the opinion change. Based on cognitive dissonance theory, which was proposed by Leon Festinger in 1957, a person experiences dissonance when s/he finds the communicator's opinion is discrepant from what s/he has in mind. But, how do people get rid of or reduce the dissonance? Aronson, Turner, and Carlsmith (1963) suggested two modes: the recipient may reduce dissonance by changing his/her own opinion or by derogating the communicator. Their experiment tried to find out: Under what conditions, will one change one's opinion and/or derogate the communicator when an individual is exposed at various discrepant circumstances? The key point is the communicator's credibility. Aronson et a1. (1963) assumed that (1) when the communicator is highly credible, the larger the degree of opinion change advocated, the greater the dissonance, and the greater the opinion change. (2) As the communicator is presented as mildly credible, derogation will replace 2 opinion change as a method to reduce dissonance. Accordingly, there would be little or no opinion change. Their results showed that there is a significant relationship between communication discrepancy and opinion change. Under the condition of the highly credible communicator, the greater the discrepancy between the communicator's opinion and the opinion of the recipient, the greater opinion change. That is, opinion change increases with degree of discrepancy in the high credibility level. However, in the mildly credible condition, it produces greater opinion change with increasing discrepancy only up to a point. The experiment designed by Bochner and Insko (1966) extended the results of Aronson et a1. (1963). It measured three dependent variables: opinion regarding sleep, disparagement of the communication, and disparagement of the source. It manipulated three independent variables: discrepancy (nine levels of hours of sleep advocated), source credibility (high or medium), and order of dependent- variable measurement (opinion-disparagement or disparagement-opinion). The findings confirmed, in one hand, the study of Aronson et a1. (1963) that there was significant curvilinearity between opinion change and discrepancy for the medium credibility source. The expectation that the high credibility source would show a curvilinear trend, however, 3 was not supported by the data. In the high credibility condition, opinion change was found to be linearly related to discrepancy between the communicator and recipients. Also, the data showed that the medium and high credibility sources do not differ in opinion change at the moderate discrepancy levels since theoretically the high credibility source should have been equal to or superior to the medium credibility source. Therefore, if the source is highly credible, the relationship between discrepancy and opinion change reveals linearity. If the source is moderately credible, the relationship tends to be curvilinear. The above studies treated the communicator's (source) credibility as the important factor to affect recipients' attitude change when they experience discrepancy. But, what if recipients found that what they expected from the source disconfirm the source's actual position? Eagly, Wood, and Chaiken (1978) studied the effect of disconfirmation on attitude change. They suggested that recipients would be influenced by two types of communicator biases during the process of message persuasiveness: knowledge bias and reporting bias. Knowledge bias refers to a recipient's belief that a communicator's knowledge about external reality is not truthful because of personal attributes. Reporting bias refers to the belief that a communicator's willingness to bring an accurate message of 4 external reality is compromised since s/he suffers situational pressures. The results indicated that the communicator gained in perceived sincerity by taking the expected position in the knowledge-bias conditions since the source's disposition corresponded to his behavior. In contrast, in the reporting- bias conditions, the communicator was regarded as insincere when he advocated a position that confirmed subjects' expectancies. However, the source was judged more biased when he confirmed rather than disconfirmed expectations. No matter whether the source disconfirmed a knowledge-bias or reporting-bias expectancy (or both), he was significantly more persuasive than when expectancies were disconfirmed. Wood and Eagly (1981) also studied persuasive messages through attribution analysis. They manipulated subjects' initial position, expectancy confirmation, and discrepancy of the source advocated position as three independent variables. As for dependent variables, they measured causal attributions, communicator bias, message comprehension, and opinion change. The findings of this study asserted that both perceived communicator bias and message comprehension were determinants of opinion change. When the communicator's position disconfirmed subjects' expectancies based on the communicator's background, he was perceived as unbiased and more persuasive since recipients believed that he was 5 influenced by factual evidence. This result confirms the earlier study of Eagly et al. (1978) that message disconfirmation leads to greater opinion change. From the above studies, it is obvious that two factors influencing opinion change are message discrepancy and message disconfirmation. Studies have defined message discrepancy as the difference between the position advocated by the source and the initial position of the recipient. Message disconfirmation has been defined by Kaplowitz and Fink (1990) as the difference between the position advocated by the source and the position which the recipient expected from the source.1 The effects of discrepancy and disconfirmation on opinion change seem, however, to have been confounded by many studies. Do both of them have distinct effects on opinion change from each other? Kaplowitz et al.'s (1990) study has further clarified the relationship between discrepancy and disconfirmation. First of all, they specify that discrepancy studies typically use positional measures, but disconfirmation studies do not. Also, results of Wood and Eagly (1981) and Nemecek (1985)'s findings, suggested that discrepancy and 1Discrepancy and disconfirmation have been defined as follows: Dp = PA- P0 Df=PA-PE where Up is discrepancy; Df is disconfirmation; P’ is the position advocated by the source; P’ is the recipient's initial position, and.Pg is the p051tion expected from the source. 6 disconfirmation affect different aspects of the receiver's opinion regarding the attitude object, via different processes. Thus, they tested a model, the two process - two opinion model (see Figure 1), which assumes that discrepancy directly affects the receiver's final position and disconfirmation directly affect the receiver's comparative evaluation of the current issue relative to others in some class. Kaplowitz et al. (1990) created the questionnaire booklet which described the sentence guideline for armed robbery, Judge Walter's background and his past judicial experience, the case of Convict X, and, then, asked subjects' opinions about criminal issues. The results of the two experiments successfully disentangle the effects of discrepancy and disconfirmation on attitude change and support the two process-two opinion model that discrepancy has a strong direct effect on final position, whereas disconfirmation has a direct effect on comparative evaluation. In these two process, what is the role of cognitive elaboration? Kaplowitz et al. (1990) concluded that subjects responded to varying levels of discrepancy by simply taking a weighted average of their initial position and the position advocated, without deeply engaging in cognitive elaboration. Elaboration, however, is required for the indirect effect of disconfirmation on final position. Figurel- Initial Position Discrepancy Disconfirmation / / Final Position Comparative Evaluation 8 Therefore, Kaplowitz et al. (1990) suggested that the effects of disconfirmation need a greater degree of cognitive elaboration and future research should manipulate and measure elaboration levels to see the results under various conditions of cognitive elaboration. The current study has taken the above suggestion in consideration to do further research. The amount of thinking (elaboration) is manipulated as the third independent variable in addition to discrepancy and disconfirmation. Hypotheses According to Kaplowitz et al.'s study (1990), the two process model implies that focus of attention can moderate the effect of disconfirmation on comparative evaluation. They suggested that the greater when subjects were asked to focus on the position expected from the source, there was the greater the effect of disconfirmation on comparative evaluation than when subjects were asked to focus on the issue. Based on this, we hypothesize - H1) The more subjects think about the source, the greater should be the effect of disconfirmation on subjects' comparative evaluation of the current case, that is, the source focus should induce the effect of disconfirmation on comparative evaluation. 9 Moreover, the results of Kaplowitz et al's (1990) prior experiments showed that for comparative evaluation and position to affect each other, cognitive elaboration was required. In the first experiment, the issue focus group indicated the larger number of thoughts than the source focus group. Similarly, the second experiment showed that subjects, who had received a large amount of knowledge about the source's previous behavior, had more thoughts (see Kaplowitz et al. 1990). These two groups, but not the others, showed significant effects of comparative evaluation on final position. Accordingly, there seem to be a larger effects of comparative evaluation on subjects' final position when recipients think more about the issue under considerations, i.e. when they cognitively elaborate on message contents. If this is the case, then, we have hypothesis 2 - H2) The greater the amount of cognitive elaboration, on the issue, the greater will be the effect of comparative evaluation on final position. Hypothesis 1 emphasizes source focus, which is thought to increase the effect of disconfirmation on comparative evaluation. Hypothesis 2 stresses issue focus, which is thought to enhance the effect of comparative evaluation on final position. If (1) and/or (2) above are correct, this would lead to an indirect positive effect of disconfirmation on position. Method Overview This study employed a 2 (source focus encouraged vs. not) x 2 (issue focus encouraged vs. not) x 3 (discrepancy levels: small, moderate, or extreme) x 3 (disconfirmation levels: small, moderate, or extreme) factorial design. Subjects were told that we were doing a study of public attitudes regarding the criminal justice system, and were given a questionnaire booklet to complete. The Experimental Design This study continued to use the questionnaire booklet similar to one designed by Kaplowitz et a1. (1990), which concerned public attitudes towards the criminal justice system. The topic of criminal sentencing was chosen because it can express the message and subjects' position on a numerical scale with ratio measurement. Armed robbery was selected from 16 different crimes based on a pilot study in which the recommended years imprisonment for armed robbery had the lowest dispersion relative to its central tendency (Kaplowitz et al. 1990). First of all, the questionnaire begins with the introduction of criminal justice system and its functions 10 11 and the purpose of this study. Subjects then are given some information about the Michigan sentencing guideline for the crime of armed robbery, which is ten years imprisonment. Based on the sentencing policies, subjects give their initial views of the appropriate sentence for the crime of armed robbery, which is treated as the initial position (Po). Next, subjects were told a story about Judge Walters, who has served as a judge for several years. During his years as a judge, Judge Walters had sentenced many defendants, often relying on his own points of view, instead of simply relying on the sentencing guidelines. Subjects were informed that Judge Walters had imposed fifteen prison sentences for armed robbery and were informed of the average, the smallest, and the greatest of these sentences in prison. After reading the above descriptions, subjects of the source focus group (H & S forms) were asked to write down their viewpoints about Judge Walters' degree of severity or leniency. Then, a particular defendant called Convict X, was introduced to subjects. They read the statement Judge Walters made as he sentenced Convict X. Convict X's sentence was varied in different conditions. Then, subjects indicated their final position regarding Convict X's proper sentence and also answered other questions about it. 12 Independent Variables There are three independent variables in this study - message discrepancy, message disconfirmation, and cognitive elaboration. In order to create low, moderate, and extreme message discrepancy and message disconfirmation, we varied the position advocated by the source (PR) to be 10, 22.5, and 50 years, and the position expected from the source (Pd was manipulated by varying the previous sentences of the source for armed robbery. These were 2, 4.5, 10, 22.5, and 50 years, respectively. The individual subject's initial position (P5) should be the sentencing guideline - 10 years. Hence, a position advocated of ten years leads to a zero level of message discrepancy.2 The nine (3x3) combinations, in which discrepancy and disconfirmation are orthogonal, are presented in Table 1. 2We used the logarithms to express three levels and kinds of discrepancy and disconfirmation instead of the raw numbers. According to Kaplowitz et al. (1990), the constant ratio should be made to increase discrepancy and disconfirmation in steps which will be equally far apart to subjects. Thus, the logarithms is used. Since the lowest and highest number is 10 and 50, respectively, they can be expressed like 10, 10X, and 10X2. Since, 10x2 = 50, x = 2.24 Therefore, Dp = logzz,‘ (PA/P0) = 1n (PA/Po)/ In (2.24) Similarly, Df = logzz,’ (PA/PE) = in (P /PE)/ ln (2.24) From the above steps, three levels 0 d1screpancy and disconfirmation (0, 1, 2) are created. 13 Table 1 - Discrepancy and Disconfirmation Levels for the Experimental Design I Disconfirmation level Discrepancy level ————————_ I O 1 2 0 10/10 10/4.5 10/2 (8, 12.5) (3.6, 5.6) (1.6, 2.5) 1 22.5/22.5 22.5/50 22.5/4.5> (18, 28) (8, 12.5) (3.6, 4.5) 2 50/50 50/22.5 50/10 (40, 62.5) (18, 28) (8, 12.5) Note: The number of the right of each slash is the average of previous sentences (P’) which manipulates expected position. The number of the left of each slash is the sentence for Convict X (P3). The numbers in parentheses are the lowest and highest of previous sentences for armed robbery. The initial position.(Pb) of the sentence for armed robbery is assumed to be 10 years. The third independent variable, which is the main different feature from Kaplowitz et al.'s study (1990), is the amount and kind of thinking (cognitive elaboration) encouraged. We designed four forms to manipulate the levels of cognitive elaboration. Four forms (H, S, I, L) are distinguished from each other in terms of specific key instructions at the end of the introduction to the form. Simultaneously, two different kinds of focuses (on source vs. on issue) were encouraged within four forms. For the definitions of these forms and what the abbreviations, H, S, I, and L stand for, see Table 2. 14 Table 2 - Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Yes H I No S L Note: H - High: Cognitive elaboration encouraged on both the source and the issue L'"HUJ I Cognitive elaboration encouraged on the source only Cognitive elaboration encouraged on the issue only Low: Cognitive elaboration encouraged neither on the source nor on the issue Two of the forms (H & 8 forms), asked subjects to think about the source, i.e. the judge. The H & I forms asked subjects to pay attention to the crime, the sentence, and the reasoning behind the sentence. As for the low level of elaboration (L form), we ask subjects to give their first impressions without carefully thinking about other things. The key instructions of H and L forms are stated, respectively, as follows: For H form - "The Commission is interested in the reasoned opinions of a broad spectrum of the community. It is especially interested in your thoughts about how the characteristics of the judge, as well as the crime and the reasoning behind the sentence, can affect the sentence. ........ Be sure to focus primarily on thegiudqe. the crime. the sentence, and the reasoning behind the sentence 15 and try not to let other things distract you. This is not a speed test." For L form - "Sometimes people form their opinions after a great deal of thought. At other times, they must act on relatively fast first impressions. While first impressions are sometimes different from judgments formed from extensive thought, the evidence suggests that they are sometimes more valid. We suspect that this may even be in true issues of criminal sentencing. Hence, we would like you to answer this questionnaire with your first impressions." In the S form, the instructions put stress on the judge and ask for subjects' thoughts about "how the characteristics of the judge can affect the sentence". In the I form, we ask subjects think about "the crime, the sentence, and the reasoning behind the sentence". In both conditions in which source focus is encouraged (S & H), subjects were also be asked their view of Judge Walters' severity, his future sentences for armed robbery, and to indicate their confidence in their own prediction. These questions, however, are omitted in the I and L forms. 16 Major Dependent Variables Subjects' attitudes were measured in two ways: the subjects' final position was their view of the sentence Convict X should receive after reading Judge Walters' sentencing speech for Convict X. The comparative evaluation of Convict X, asked how bad he was. It used a magnitude scaling question (see Lodge 1981 for discussion of this technique) which rated 100 as bad as the average armed robber. The higher the numerical value, the worse is subjects' comparative evaluation of Convict X. The scale has no maximum value. We also assume that thinking about the object should be reflected in thoughts listed. Subjects were then asked to list all thoughts about the sentence of Convict X. This thought listing was also used to measure different kinds of elaboration. In addition, we assumed that the more subjects think about the object, the more effort and time they should spend on it. We measured two different time, one is the time subjects say they spent on thinking about the case of Convict X; the other is the actual time they spent finishing the whole questionnaire. Data Collection Three hundred forty-eight male and female undergraduates in several communication courses at Michigan State University participated as subjects who received extra 17 credit for their participation. We have 36 different forms, which were randomly administered to subjects in order to get as near an equal number as possible in each form. Before subjects started to fill out the questionnaire, we explained some of the opinion scales to assist them with the magnitude scaling questions. We set the number 100 as a moderate value and subjects were free to choose any number they like. In other words, there was no upper limit. To enhance motivation, subjects were told that their opinions would be important to judges and be reported to the US Commission on Judicial Reform. They were encouraged to answer every question as well as possible. So that we could measure the amount of time spent on the questionnaire, subjects were asked to write down their time of starting on the cover sheet and their time of completion on the final page. For this purpose, we helped them by putting the time on the blackboard and updating it every minute. After excluding some subjects who have previously heard or participated or did not believe in the cover story, the final valid sample is reduced from 348 to 331. Results Data Transformations By examining the skewness and the distribution of the residuals of raw data in this study, we found that the l8 assumption of normality and homoscedasticity appeared to be violated. Therefore, we transformed the raw data to meet the assumptions. Since the distribution of residuals of the data has a tail in the positive direction, it's helpful to take logs of the dependent variables. Zero is the lowest value among the magnitude scale variables and we cannot use a log transformation without adding a constant value. Thus, we tried some different additive constants and selected the most appropriate one which had the minimal skewness.3 The data appeared to less skewed and more homoscedastic after taking the logarithm (See Table 3 & 4). 3For the variable of EFFORT, one outlier was found and we recoded the extreme values greater than 1500 to 1500 before transforming. 19 Table 3 - Skewness for Dependent Variables Dependent Skew before Skew after Variables Transformation Transformation Value of K INIT POS 2.514 -.213 0 FIN POS 2.158 .178 0 SURPRISE 3.627 -.635 15 PSYDISC 2.302 -.772 15 UNEXPECT 2.877 -.636 15 EFFORT 6.483 .480 15 BADPRE 5.419 -.396 25 CONBAD 2.975 .571 .001 TIMSPD 1.095 -.l86 l TOTATIM 3.745 .691 l Note: The logarithmically transformed data used the formula X* = Ln(X+K) where X is the raw ‘variable and X' is the transformed. The adjusted geometric mean is found by first taking the anti-logarithm of the mean of X*, then, subtracting the constant K. Here, the value K of each variable is listed on the last column as the above. For the abbreviations, INIT POS and FIN POS are subjects' initial and final positions, respectively. SURPRISE - "how surprising did you find the Judge's sentence on Convict X"; PSYDISC - "how different was the Judge's sentence on Convict X from the prior sentence"; UNEXPECT - "how unexpected did you find the Judge's sentence on Convict X"; EFFORT - "how much effort did you spend thinking about this case"; BADPRE - "how bad are the previous armed robbers"; CONBAD - "how bad is Convict X"; TIMSPD - "the time spent thinking about the case of Convict X"; TOTATIM - "the time spent completing the whole questionnaire". 20 Table 4 - Homoscedasticity for Dependent Variables Dependent Cochran's C Cochran's C Sample Variables before after Size Transformation Transformation INIT POS .2148 .1343 328 FIN POS .3387 .1994 330 BADPRE .5009 .1742 331 CONBAD .2906 .1420 331 SURPRISE .2900 .1609 331 PSYDISC .2592 .1917 330 UNEXPECT .4123 .1746 331 EFFORT .5082 .1916 331 TIMSPD .2024 .1307 330 TOTATIM .3160 .2000 331 Note: The value of Cochran's C (max variance/sum of variances), which was used to test for homoscedasticity, was computed by nine group (three levels of discrepancy * three levels of disconfirmation) in ANOVA. Manipulation Checks Discrepancy - Our assumption that a position advocated of ten years has a discrepancy of zero requires that subjects initial position (P5) be that ten years is the proper sentence for armed robbery. In other words, I3 should have a mean of 10 years and low variance. The geometric mean of subjects' initial attitude was 10.87 years. There were 37.2% of subjects choosing ten years and 89.1% of subjects in the range from five to fifteen years. 21 Also, we checked the correlation between PSYDISC (how psychological discrepant Judge Walters' sentence was) and the actual positional discrepancy, r=.429, F(1, 328)=73.911, p=.000. Thus, subjects were aware of the discrepancy. Disconfirmation - It was manipulated by varying both the position Judge Walters was expected to pass and the actual position he decided to sentence Convict X. After reading Judge Walters' average sentences in the previous armed robbery (PREVAVE), subjects of source focus group (H & S forms) were asked about expected average sentence from Judge Walters' in the future. We found that PREVAVE predicted subjects' expectation of Judge Walters' average sentence in the future (FUTAVEl) with r=.843, F(1, 112)=272.887, p=.000. It showed that subjects expected Judge Walters to behave the same way as he had behaved before. For the manipulation of disconfirmation, both of magnitude scaling questions - SURPRISE and UNEXPECT (how surprising and how unexpected Convict X's sentence was) were significantly affected by Df. For Df and SURPRISE, analysis of variance showed that, r=.503, F(1, 329)=111.662, p=.000: for UNEXPECT, r=.495, F(1, 329)=106.660, p=.000. The results mean that subjects responded to the two questions according to the different levels of disconfirmation. 22 Cognitive Elaboration - There were two independent cognitive focus variables, source focus and issue focus, which were presented by four forms. Six variables were measured as manipulation check: subjects' effort spent thinking about the case of Convict X (EFFORT); their report time on this case (TIMSPD): their total number thoughts while completing this questionnaire (TOTTH) - source thoughts (SOTH) and issue thoughts (ISTH); and their time finishing the whole questionnaire (TOTATIM). If manipulation of elaboration is successful, we would expect to see either the source or the issue focus groups (H, S, & I forms) spend significantly more effort, and more time, and generate more thoughts than the group of L form. We would also expect more source thoughts in the source focus conditions and more issue thoughts in the issue focus conditions. In other words, the main or interaction effects of the two elaboration encouragement on the above six dependent variables would be anticipated. For EFFORT (see Table 5), we found that there was no significant effort of source focus (F=.168, p=.683) or issue focus (F=.116, p=.734) since subjects in L form contributed more effort (M=4.97) than those in H form (M=4.93). 23 Table 5 - The Means of Subjects' Effort Spent Thinking about Convict x Case (EFFORT) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Row means Yes 4.93 (H) 5.01 (I) 4.97 No 5.00 (S) 4.97 (L) 4.99 Column means 4.97 4.99 4.98 Note: The variable of EFFORT has been transformed prior to doing ANOVA. There are between 73 and 83 subjects per cell, for a total N=308. As for TIMSPD, though subjects of both elaboration conditions did spend more time on the current case on the average (Table 6), it failed to show significant effects of either of the two independent variables. For source focus, F=1.286, p=.258, and for issue focus, F=1.378, p=.241. Table 6 - The Means of Subjects' Time Spent on the Case of Convict x (TIMSPD) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Row means Yes 2.23 (H) 2.13 (I) 2.18 No 2.08 (S) 2.09 (L) 2.08 Column means 2.15 2.11 2.13 Note: The unit of time is ndnutes. TIMSPD was transformed before doing ANOVA. There are between 80 and 85 subjects per cell, for a total N=330. 24 As another check on the focus manipulation, subjects were asked to list all thoughts in their minds. The results indicated that subjects in either the source or issue focus groups generated more thoughts than those in L form (Table 7), however, the slight difference of the means of total thoughts was not statistically significant since the values of F for source focus and issue focus are .028 (p=.867) and .235 (p=.628), respectively. Table 7 - The Means of Subjects' Total Thoughts on the Whole Questionnaire (TOTTH) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Row means Yes 2.68 (H) 2.79 (I) 2.74 No 2.66 (S) 2.64 (L) 2.65 Column means 2.67 2.71 2.69 Note: There are about 73 to 83 subjects per cell, and the total N=308. Among total thoughts, we distinguished two kinds of thinking - source thoughts and issue thoughts. There were significant effects of source focus and issue focus on these two dependent variables. For the manipulation of source thoughts (SOTH), Table 8 showed that subjects of source focus group engaged in more significantly source thinking, F(1, 327)=6.102, p=.014. We also saw that an issue focus actually reduced the number of source thoughts, 25 F(1, 327)=31.464, p=.000. In addition, there was a significant source focus by issue focus interactions on SOTH, F(1, 330)=11.920, p=.001. Table 8 - The Means of Subjects' Source Thoughts (80TH) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Row means Yes .20 (H) .29 (I) .25 No .93 (S) .46 (L) .69 Column means .57 .38 .47 Note: There are about 80 to 85 subjects per cell, and total N=331. For the issue thoughts (ISTH), significant effects of both source focus and issue focus groups were found, F(1, 330)=6.036, p=.015, and F(1, 330)=12.035, p=.001, respectively. Table 9 indicates the result. While an issue focus increased the number of issue thoughts, it decreased the number of source thoughts. 26 Table 9 - The Means of Subjects' Issue Thoughts (ISTH) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Row means Yes 2.13 (H) 2.31 (I) 2.22 No 1.35 (S) 1.96 (L) 1.66 Column means 1.74 2.13 1.94 Note: The number of subjects is from 80 to 85 per cell, and total N=331. Finally, for the time of completion of this questionnaire, there was a highly significant effect of source focus variable (H & 8 forms) on TOTATIM, r=.242, F(1, 330)=20.464, p=.000. It indicated that subjects in the source focus conditions spent more time on the whole questionnaire. There were also two-way interaction effects of source and issue focus, F(1, 330)=6.322, p=.012. (see Table 10). 27 Table 10 — The Means of Subjects' Actual Time Spent on Finishing the Whole Questionnaire (TOTATIM) by Cognitive Elaboration Condition Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Row means Yes 3.13 (H) 3.07 (I) 3.10 No 3.21 (S) 3.01 (L) 3.11 Column means 3.17 3.04 3.11 Note: The unit of time is minutes. TOTATIM was transformed. The number of subjects is from 80 to 85 per cell, and total N=330. In sum, it appeared that both SOTH and ISTH variables were manipulated successfully for source focus and issue focus, and TOTATIM indicated success only for source focus manipulation. Effects on Attitude Change Four way analysis of covariance was performed to examine the effects of independent variables on attitude change, which were measured by subjects' final position (PH and comparative evaluations of Convict X. The independent variables include discrepancy, disconfirmation, source focus, issue focus, and subjects' initial position (P3) as the covariate. For subjects' final position (Pg), we found highly significant effects of the covariate P0,IF(1, 327)=145.08, p=.000, and Discrepancy, F(2, 327)=69.254, p=.000. It showed 28 that there was a strong linear relationship between discrepancy and attitude change (final position), but disconfirmation had a non-significant relationship with position, consisted with Kaplowitz et al.'s two opinion-two process model. See Table 11 for the geometric mean and 95% confidence interval of subjects' recommended sentences for Convict X, by discrepancy and disconfirmation level. Table 11 - Geometric Mean (95% Confidence Interval) of Subjects' Final Position for Convict X (P1) by Discrepancy and Disconfirmation Level Discrepancy Disconfirmation level level 0 1 2 Row means 0 11.02 9.97 10.87 10.56 (9.44,12.22) (8.5,11.69) (9.72,12.16) l 15.56 13.63 14.06 14.47 (13.59,17.83) (12.3,15.1) (12.23,16.17) 2 21.37 20.41 20.68 20.81 (17.43,26.23) (l7.32,24.05) (17.15,24.95) Column 15.67 14.76 15.32 15.27 means Note: The geometric mean is the anti-logarithm of the arithmetic mean of the logarithmically transformed data. The limits of the confidence interval for the geometric mean are obtained by taking anti—logarithms of the limits of the confidence interval for the transformed data. There are between 32 and 42 subjects per cell, and the total sample is 330. For the comparative evaluation of the badness of Convict X (CONBAD), however, only the independent variable of Df showed a highly significant effect, F(2, 327)=16.240, p=.000. The means for low, disconfirmation are; moderate, 119.4, 29 and extreme 166.9, and 174.8, respectively. It indicated that subjects' comparative evaluation of the badness of Convict X increased with the rising of disconfirmation levels (see Table 12). Table 12 - Adjusted Geometric Evaluation of Convict X's Badness by Discrepancy and Disconfirmation.Level Means (95% Confidence Interval) of Comparative Discrepancy Disconfirmation level I level 0 l 0 125.3 159.2 145 142.1 (105,149.5) (136.6,185.7) (117,180) 1 123.8 175.4 140.8 144.9 (107.4,142.6) (141.7,216.8) (118.6,167) 2 107.3 166.7 236.3 171.9 (90 3,127.7) (135.8,204.4) (192 5,289.7) Column means 119.4 166.9 174.8 149 Note: For a definition of the adjusted geometric mean, see Table 3 (Here, K-.001). There are between 32 and 42 subjects per cell, for a total N-330. 100 is a moderately bad level, and the higher the value, the worse Convict X is viewed. Tests of Hypotheses As for the effects of cognitive elaboration, subjects using the H & S forms should think more about the source than those using the other forms. This should lead to a greater effect of disconfirmation on comparative evaluation of the Convict X case. The result clearly confirms 30 the greater should be the effect of disconfirmation on subjects' comparative evaluation of the current case (see Table 13). Table 13 - The Effect of Disconfirmation on Comparative Evaluation Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Yes 8=.315** (H) p=.155 (I) No 6=.375*** (S) B=.156 (L) Note: Simple regression was run with comparative evaluation as the dependent variable and disconfirmation as the predictor. Beta (6) is the standardized coefficient. ** p<.01, and *** p<.001. On the other hand, we predicted that a greater issue focus should increase the greater effect of comparative evaluation on final position. This second hypothesis was confirmed by subjects of I form (the issue focus group), who showed a significant effect of comparative evaluation on final position (see Table 14). That is, the greater the amount of cognitive elaboration subjects engage in the issue, the greater is the effect of comparative evaluation on final position. 31 Table 14 - The Effect of Comparative Evaluation on Final Position Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Yes 6=.097 (H) 6:.166" (I) No 6:.080 (S) 8:.070 (L) Note: Multiple regression was run with final position as the dependent variable and discrepancy, initial position, and comparative evaluation as predictors. * p<.05. If disconfirmation (Df) significantly affects comparative evaluation (Eval), and comparative evaluation also has a significant effect on final position (IQ), then, it should lead to the relationship that disconfirmation indirectly affects final position. However, none of the four forms indicated such indirect positive effect of disconfirmation on final position.“ According to cognitive consistency theories, subjects' comparative evaluation of Convict X's badness and their final position regarding the appropriate sentence should be related to each other. Based on Kaplowitz et al's finding (1990), we would expect that comparative evaluation should ‘The diagram is like as follows: Df 4 Eval prep, Eval » P1 Though H & S forms showed strong effects of Df on Eval, they did not have any significant effect of Eval on.P’. Thus, the indirect positive effect of Df on PH is always small. 32 have a greater effect on position than vice versa. Hypothesis 2 has predicted that subjects' comparative evaluation affect their final position. But, conversely, their final position can also predict comparative evaluation (see Table 15). This result is not expected because the finding of Kaplowitz et al. (1990) seems to be an one way effect. This study, however, shows two-way effects instead of one way. It seems that position has more impact on comparative evaluation than the reverse, at least in our study. However, these ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions are somewhat questionable because the causal model has two-way causation. Table 15 - The Effect of Final Position on Comparative Evaluation Issue Source Focus Focus Yes No Yes 6=.220* (H) ,8=.239* (I) No 6=.083 (S) 6=.262* (L) Note: Multiple regression was run with comparative evaluation as the dependent variable and. disconfirmation and final position as predictors. * p<.05. Discussion and Conclusion Consistent with Kaplowitz et al.'s study (1990), the results of this study confirmed the assumptions of the two process model that discrepancy had a strong and direct effect on final position and disconfirmation had a significantly direct effect on comparative evaluation of the current case. The results also provided strong support for hypothesis 1 that the more subjects focus about the source, the greater the effect of disconfirmation on their comparative evaluation. Hypothesis 2 was confirmed partly by one of the issue focus groups (I form), which showed a significant effect of comparative evaluation on final position. Originally, we would expect that the source or issue focus conditions should encourage subjects' to spend more effort and time, and to produce more thoughts. Since the manipulation of elaboration by these variables did not work very successfully, it influenced the experimental result. We suspect that some errors in the experimental forms make such difference. Within the four forms, the first paragraph on page 44 (see Appendix), which introduces Judge Walter's sentence for Convict X, should not be the same in 33 34 the four focus groups. This error may have had some effect on the result. In addition, through the manipulation of source focus and issue focus, we asked subjects think about the source, but it seemed that may not be enough because one kind of thinking (source) induced the other kind of thinking (issue), which weakened the effects of comparative evaluation on final position. One important question about cognitive elaboration remains unaddressed by the present study. It concerns the limitations on increased the issue focus facilitating message processing. Theoretically, characteristics of the issue content should have a greater impact under high cognitive elaboration conditions. If the issue, however, is not so important and is non-self—relevant to subjects, there would be some limitations to enhance subjects' issue focus even in the high cognitive elaboration condition (Petty and Cacioppo, 1979). It may provide one of the reasons that subjects of the issue focus group of this study did not show strong effects of their comparative evaluation on final position since they were not highly involved in the issue of criminal justice system. LIST OF REFERENCES REFERENCES Aronson, E., Turner, J.A., and Carlsmith, J.M. 1963 "Communicator Credibility and Communication Discrepancy as Determinants of Opinion Change". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67: 31—36. Bochner, S., and Insko, C.A. 1966 "Communicator Discrepancy, Source Credibility, and Opinion Change". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4: 614-621. Chaiken, Shelly 1980 "Heuristic versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source versus Message Cues in Persuasion". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39: 752- 766. Chaiken, S., and Stangor, C. 1987 "Attitudes and Attitude Change". Annual Review PSYChOlOgy 38: 575-630. 35 36 Chaiken, Shelly 1987 "The Heuristic Model of Persuasion". In M.P. Zanna, J.M. Olson, and C.P. Herman (Eds.), Social Influences: The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 5, Pp. 3-39. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eagly, A.H., Wood, W., and Chaiken, S. 1978 "Causal Inferences about Communicators and Their Effect on Opinion Change". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36: 424-435. Festinger, L., and Aronson, E. 1960 "The Arousal and Reduction of Dissonance in Social Contexts". In D. Cartwright & A. Zander (Eds.), Group Dynamics: Research and Theory. (2nd ed.), Pp. 214-231. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson. Hovland, C.I., Harvey, O.J., and Sherif, M. 1957 "Assimilation and Contrast Effects in Reactions to Communication and Attitude Change". Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology 55: 244-252. Johnson, B.T., and Eagly, A.H. 1989 "Effects of Involvement on Persuasion: A Meta- Analysis". Psychological Bulletin 106: 290-314. 37 Kaplowitz, S.A., Fink, L., Mulcrone, J., Atkin, D., and Dabil, S. 1990 "Disentangling the Effects of Discrepancy and Disconfirming Information". Unpublished paper, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Lodge, M. 1981 Magnitude Scaling: Quantitative Measurement of Opinions. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Nemecek, P.J. 1985 "The Effects of Disconfirmation and Discrepancy on Attitude Change". Unpublished master's thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Petty, R.E., and Cacioppo, J.T. 1979 "Issue Involvement Can Increase or Decrease Persuasion by Enhancing Message - Relevant Cognitive Responses". Journal of Personality and Social Psycholoqy 37: 1915- 1926. Petty, R.E., Cacioppo, J.T., and Goldman, R. 1981 "Personal Involvement as a Determinant of Argument- Based Persuasion". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41: 847-855. 38 Petty, R.E., and Cacioppo, J.T. 1981 Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches. Dubuque, IA: Wm.C. Brown. Petty, R.E., and Cacioppo, J.T. 1986 Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer-Verlag. Wood, W., and Eagly, A.H. 1981 "Stages in the Analysis of Persuasive Messages: The Role of Causal Attributions and Message Comprehension". Journal of Personality and Social Psycholoqy 40: 246- 259. APPENDIX CARD 1 SID Study of Public Attitudes Towards the Criminal Justice System Michigan State University Form 22H 1990 Today's date Course Number Section Number Class Time Time Started Questionnaire 40 Study of Public Attitudes Towards the Criminal Justice System Crime has long been a major concern of our society, but different people have different views as to the prOper treatment of criminals. Some people feel that our legal system treats criminals too leniently to be an effective deterrent. Others, however, feel that time in prison makes criminals more likely to commit crimes in the future and that overly long terms require the state to spend too much of its money on prisons. If the law is to be supported, people must believe that the criminal justice system serves the needs of society. While not everyone need agree with every judicial decision, it is necessary that the public understand the way in which the criminal justice system functions and the kinds of factors which can enter into sentencing decisions. In this study, you. will first. be asked about. your' own experience with the criminal justice system and your acquaintance with people who work in this system. You will then be given some information about sentencing policies in Michigan and asked your initial views of the proper sentence for a crime. You will then be told of a real-life sentencing decision by a judge and will be asked some questions about this decision. The answers you provide will help make judges more aware of your concerns since your answers will be reported to the US Commission. on. Judicial Reform. 'This body is currently studying sentencing policies and practices in various states and will make recommendations about them. The Commission is interested in the reasoned opinions of a broad spectrum of the community. It is especially interested in your thoughts about how the characteristics of the judge, as well as the crime and the reasoning behind the sentence, can affect the sentence. 'Fherefore, please read all questions carefully and give each of them the carefully thought out answers they deserve. Be sure to focus primarily on Q1; iudqe1,the crime. the sentence. and the reasoning behind the sentence and try not to let other things distract you. This is not a speed test. We are aware that you do not have full information about the case you will evaluate, but many decision in life must be made without complete information. Please answer all questions on the form. 41 Part I Below you will be asked some questions about your experience with the criminal justice system. Please answer them honestly. Be assured that your responses are totally anonymous and confidential. 1. Are you personally acquainted with anyone who has been a judge in criminal trials? Yes (1) No (0) 2. Are you personally acquainted with anyone who has been an attorney in criminal trials? Yes (1) No (0) 3. Are you personally acquainted with anyone who has been a member of a parole board? Yes (1) No (0) 4. Have you, or anyone you know well, ever been a witness in a criminal trial? Yes (1) No (0) 5. Have you or anyone you know well, ever been a defendant in a criminal trial? Yes (1) No (0) 42 Part II Sentencing Guidelines The State of Michigan, along with many other states, has issued a Sentencing Guidelines Manual. These Guidelines, which are based on a consensus of legal experts, are to assist judges and provide some degree of consistency in sentencing. Below is a copy of the cover of the Sentencing Guidelines Manual of the State of Michigan. State of Michigan The sentence you will be examining was for the crime of armed robbery. The Michigan Sentencing Guideline for the crime of -armed robbery is 10 years imprisonment. The sentence for this crime is not only a consensus of legal experts, but has also been found to be supported by a large majority of the public. These Guidelines, however, are recommendations, not laws. Because many people feel that a judge must be able to take into account the special features of each case, the law permits a judge to pass a sentence which is considerably greater or considerably less than the Guideline. To make sure that you have absorbed all of the information above, please answer the following questions. REFER TO THE INFORMATION ABOVE, IF YOU NEED TO REFRESH YOUR MEMORY. 43 On the views of which group of people are the Sentencing Guidelines based? What is the Sentencing Guideline for the crime of armed robbery? years imprisonment HOW'does the public feel about this particular Guideline? Think of some reasons which might be used to justify the selection of 10 years as the Guideline for the crime of armed robbery. How many years in prison do you think is an appropriate sentence for the crime of armed robbery? years (Note: You need not choose a whole number of years but do choose a number of years. Hence, please do not choose "life imprisonment" or "death." 'The death penalty is not allowed in Michigan.) Are judges required to follow these Guidelines? ___ Yes (1) __ N0 (0) Why or why not? 44 Part III As stated earlier, while the State of Michigan has Sentencing Guidelines, the State still allows.Iudges to make up their own minds in passing sentences. “Therefore, a sentence may deviate considerably from the Guidelines, for a variety of reasons. We will now give you some information about a particular judge, whom we shall call Judge Walters. The following are excerpts from a report on various Michigan Judges, which was released one year ago. Judge Walters is a judge in one of the larger metropolitan areas in Michigan. He is in his fifties, has gray hair, is married, and has grown children. He has had many years of experience as a judge in criminal cases. In imposing sentences, he sometimes imposes the sentence recommended by the Sentencing Guidelines. However, he places the greatest weight on his own judgment. In his many years of judicial experience, Judge Walters has had to pass sentence on many defendants for a variety of crimes. . . . Of those defendants, fifteen were convicted of, and sentenced for, the crime of armed robbery. Of these fifteen.sentences.for'armed robbery, the average»sentence he imposed was about 10 years in prison. The smallest of these sentences was 8 years, while the greatest of them was 12.5 years. . . . (Criminal sentences are typically expressed as a certain number of years plus a certain number of months. To improve comprehension, however, this report has expressed parts of a year in decimal format. For example, 6 months is expressed as .5 years.) 45 Like all people, different judges have different points of view. Some tend to be more severe with defendants who have been found guilty, while others tend to be more lenient. Please answer the following questions, before you turn to the next page. 1. Keeping in mind what you have learned about Judge Walters, how severe do you view him? If Judge Walters continues to be a judge for many more years, he may pass many more sentences for armed robbery. Suppose he passes 100 more sentences for armed robbery. These 100 sentences may not all be the same. We would like to know, however, what you think will be the average of the next 100 sentences he may pass for armed robbery. 2. If, in the future, Judge Walters passes 100 additional sentences for armed robbery, what is your best guess as to what the average of the additional sentences will be? My guess is that the average of Judge Walters' future sentences for armed robbery will be years imprisonment 3. How sure do you feel about the guess you have just made? If you feel completely certain it is correct, choose 100%. If you feel completely certain it is igcorrect, choose 9%. If you feel it is equally likely to be right or wrong. choose 50%. You may use any number between 0% and 100%. I feel % sure of my answer to question 2 above. 46 Part IV This past year, after the release of the report which we quoted, Judge Walters had to again sentence someone for armed robbery. We will refer to this person as Convict X. We will now present the statement Judge Walters made as he sentenced Convict X. As you read it, keep in mind both what you know about Judge Walters the reasons he gave for his decision. By threat of force and violence, you gained access to money which was not rightfully yours. You brandished a lethal weapon and made quite clear that you would not hesitate to use it if your crime were in any way resisted. Since there was no resistance, you did not fire your weapon, but the terror you instilled in all of those present will be with them for a very long time. Clearly, you played a major role in the planning and execution of this crime. jFinally, your record shows that this is not the first time that you have violated the laws which create a civilized society. . . Therefore, I sentence you to 50 years in the penitentiary. Before preceding further, let us be sure that certain facts are clear in your mind. To assure this please take a moment to answer the following questions. YOU MAY TURN BACK TO REFRESH YOUR MEMORY IF NECESSARY. 1. What was the Sentencing Guideline for the crime of armed robbery? years imprisonment 2. What was your best guess as to the average of the sentences Judge Walters would pass in the future? years imprisonment 3. What sentence did Judge Walters pass on Convict X? years imprisonment 47 Remember it is important that you think about how the characteristics of the judge and those of the criminal and crime can influence the sentence. To help you with this thinking, please answer the following questions. 1) Taking into account what you know about Judge Walters, and about the case of Convict X, why do you think Judge Walters imposed that particular sentence on Prisoner X? 2) Do you think this was an appropriate sentence? Why or why not? You will soon be asked to evaluate the case of Convict X. .As you answer these questions, please be sure to think about 1) the Sentencing Guideline for the crime of Armed Robbery 2) Judge Walters' sentence for Convict X 3) How strict or lenient Judge Walters generally is, and the sentences he has given before and 4) the reasons Judge Walters gave for his sentence. 48 Part V Earlier in this study, you expressed an opinion about the usual sentence for armed robbery but since then, you have received information about this specific case and have had additional time to think about this issue. Therefore, please feel free to give a different answer than you gave before, if you feel that is appropriate. We know that you do not have all of the information that Judge Walters had when he imposed the sentence he didc Nonetheless, please answer the following questions the best you can. ‘While some of the questions may seem very similar to others, please answer all of them. 1. To how many years in prison do you think Convict X should have been sentenced? (Note: You need not choose a whole number of years) but do choose a number of years. Hence, please do not choose "life imprisonment " or "death." The death penalty is not allowed in Michigan.) I think Convict X should have been sentenced to years in prison. We will next be asking you some questions using a kind of scale with which you may not be familiar. While some scales have 100 as a maximum value, the scales which follow treat 100 as a moderate value and have no upper limit. 2. Think back to the sentence you expected Judge Walters to impose on Convict X, after you were introduced to Judge Walters, but before you read his sentencing speech. How surprising did you find the sentence he imposed on Convict X? If you found the sentence not at all surprising, write 0 (zero). Iijou found his sentence moderately surprising, write 100. If you found his sentence twice as surprising as moderately surprising, choose 200 (2 x 100). If you found his sentence half as surprising as moderately surprising, choose 50 (1/2 x 100). You may use any number between 0 and 100 to indicate a level of surprise which is less than moderate and any number greater than 100 for to indicate a level of surprise which is greater than moderate. While 0 is the lowest number you may use, there is no "highest number." Judge Walters' sentence of Convict X was units surprising to me. PLEASE THINK ABOUT EACH QUESTION BEFORE YOU ANSWER IT. THINK ABOUT THE JUDGE, THE SENTENCE AND THE REASONING BEHIND THE SENTENCE. 49 3. Think back to the sentence you thought armed robbery should receive, before you read Judge Walters' sentencing speech. How different was Judge Walters' sentence of Convict X from your own prior view of a proper sentence for armed robbery? Imagine that 100 is moderately different from your own view. If you think the difference between your view and Judge Walters' sentence is twice as much as a moderate difference, rate it as a 200. If you think the difference between your view and Judge Walters' view is half as much as a moderate difference, rate it as a 50. If Judge Walters' sentence is not at all different from your view, rate it Q (zero). You may use any number you wish, from zero on up. Judge Walters' sentence was units different from the sentence I initially would have imposed. 4. How expert a judge do you think Judge Walters is? (Imagine that 100 is moderately expert and zero is not at all expert.) I think Judge Walters is units expert. 5. How fair' a judge do you think; Judge Walters' is? (Imagine that 100 is moderately fair and zero is not at all fair.) I think Judge Walters is units fair. 6. How serious a crime is armed robbery? (Imagine that 100 is moderately serious and zero is not at all serious). I think armed robbery is units serious. 7. Think back to the sentence you expected Judge Walters to impose on Convict X, after you were introduced to Judge Walters, but before you read his sentencing speech. How unexpected did you find the sentence that Judge Walters imposed on Convict X? (Imagine that 100 is moderately unexpected and zero is not at all unexpected.) I found the sentence Judge Walters imposed on Convict X units unexpected. PLEASE THINK ABOUT EACH QUESTION BEFORE YOU ANSWER IT. THINK ABOUT THE JUDGE, THE SENTENCE AND THE REASONING BEHIND THE SENTENCE. 50 8. How objective a judge do you think Judge Walters is? (Imagine that 100 is moderately objective and zero is not at all objective.) I think Judge Walters is units objective. 9. How biased a judge do you think Judge Walters is? (Imagine that 100 is moderately biased and zero is not at all biased.) I think Judge Walters is units biased. 10. How strong, or compelling, do you think.were the reasons Judge Walters had for the sentence he imposed on Convict X? (Imagine that 100 is moderately compelling and zero is not at all compelling.) I thought the case for Judge Walters' sentence was units compelling. We are now going to ask you how bad you think various criminals are. To measure badness of criminals, let us now use a new "yardstick". As your new "yardstick", imagine that the average armed robber is 100 units bad and zero is not at all bad. You will then be comparing the badness of various criminals with the badness of the average armed robber. 11. Think back those who Judge Walters sentenced for armed robbery, prior to his sentence of Convict X. How bad are they? I think those‘previously sentenced for armed robbery, by.Judge Walters, were units bad. 12. How bad is Convict X? (Again, imagine that the average armed robber is 100 units bad.) I think Convict X is units bad. PLEASE THINK ABOUT EACH QUESTION BEFORE YOU ANSWER IT. THINK ABOUT THE JUDGE, THE SENTENCE AND THE REASONING BEHIND THE SENTENCE. 51 13. Imagine that -7 is strongly favoring, for Convict X, the ten year sentence recommended by the Sentencing Guidelines and +7 is strongly favoring Judge Walters' actual sentence for Convict X. Further, imagine that numbers between -7 and +7 represent views in between those extremes. Where would you put your own view on that scale? (Circle the number which best represents your View.) 10 Actual years Sentence -7 [-6 [-5 I-4 l—3 |-2 [-1 I 0 [+1 1+2 |+3 |+4 |+5 1+6 1+7 PLEASE THINK ABOUT EACH QUESTION BEFORE YOU ANSWER IT. THINK ABOUT THE JUDGE, THE SENTENCE AND THE REASONING BEHIND THE SENTENCE. 52 Part VI We would now like to ask some factual questions. Please answer them, without looking’ up the information in the questionnaire. Do not worry if your answers are incorrect. The questionnaire is strictly anonymous and any wrong answers will have no effect on you. 1. Prior to his sentence of Convict X, how many defendants did Judge Walters sentence for armed robbery? defendants 2. The average of those prior sentences was years imprisonment. 3. The lowest of those prior sentences was years. 4. The highest of those prior sentences was years. For the next few questions, place a check next to True, for statements, which are TRUE, according to the speech made by Judge Walters as he sentenced Convict X. Place a check next to False, for statements, which are FALSE, or which were not stated or implied in Judge Walters' sentencing speech. 5. In the armed robbery in which Convict X took part, someone was shot. True (1) False (0) 6. Convict X played a major role in the crime. True (1) False (0) 7. This robbery was the first crime Convict.X had committed. True (1) False (0) 8. Hostages were taken in that armed robbery. True (1) False (0) 53 Part VII We know that we have asked you many questions and some of them may seem very similar to others. We want to assure you that we do have reasons for asking them and to request your patience and co-operation in answering them. 1. Please list all thoughts which were at all related to the sentence received by Convict X. Please list each thought on a separate line. Moreover, please categorize each thought as either favoring severity (marked S), favoring leniency (marked L), or favoring neither (marked N). 2. How much effort did you spend thinking about this case? (Imagine that 100 is moderate effort and zero is no effort.) I spent units of effort on this case. 3. About hOW'mUCh time did you spend thinking about the case of Convict X? minutes and seconds 54 4. Have you previously heard of this study? Yes (1) No (0) If "yes", please explain briefly what you heard. 5. Have you previously participated in this study? ___ Yes (1) _ N0 (0) If "yes", please explain briefly. 6. In your own words, please describe the purpose of this study. 7. Please indicate your time of completion of this questionnaire. Time of Completion We very much appreciate your participation. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES llHlllmllmlll"I"llllllllllllllllmllllllllllllllllllllll 31293008810925