LIBRARY Michigan State ‘ Untverslty , PLACE IN RETURN BOX to tomovo this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES rotum on 0! before date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE L___L_____ L 4 JL m F .__7 FT 1 MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution chMHM AN OBSERVATIONAL ANALYSIS OF DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS, PERSONALITY PROFILES, AND PERCEIVED PROBLEM-SOLVING SKILLS OF ADOLESCENTS IN THERAPEUTIC FOSTER CARE BY Charlene Crickon Kushler A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling Psychology, Educational Psychology, and Special Education 1991 ABSTRACT AN OBSERVATIONAL ANALYSIS OF DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS, PERSONALITY PROFILES, AND PERCEIVED PROBLEM-SOLVING SKILLS OF ADOLESCENTS IN THERAPEUTIC FOSTER CARE BY Charlene Crickon Kushler An observational analysis was performed on 41 adolescents placed in therapeutic foster care, a relatively’ new 'treatment setting. Data. were obtained using the Millon Adolescent Personality Inventory, the Personal Change Questionnaire, and a demographic survey. Frequency data examined revealed that the majority of adolescents in therapeutic foster care were Caucasian females who were temporary wards of the court or state. More than 40% of the adolescents had a history of psychiatric or residential treatment and had been in more than one previous foster care placement. The length of placement in therapeutic foster care averaged close to 1 year. Research questions focused on the personality profiles and perceived problem-solving skills of the adolescents in that setting, as well as the relationship Charlene Crickon Kushler of those variables to the length of time in treatment. The results indicated the predominance of certain personality styles among the adolescents placed in therapeutic foster care. In particular, the adolescent females most frequently evidenced a Passive-Aggressive personality style. The second most prominent styles were equally divided among the Avoidant, Histrionic, and Compulsive personality styles. The male adolescents’ most common personality styles were Passive-Aggressive and Antisocial. A significant positive correlation was found between the Compulsive personality style and length of time in treatment. No correlation was found between the adolescents’ perceived problem-solving skills and length of time in treatment. There was a significant correlation between therapist rating of client functioning and time in treatment. Furthermore, a significant positive relation- ship existed between perceived problem-solving skills and age. Additional significant positive correlations were found between the Narcissistic and Histrionic personali- ties and certain perceived problem-solving skills. A significant negative correlation was found between the Avoidant personality and all four of the problem-solving scales. Charlene Crickon Kushler A discussion and interpretation of these findings were presented. Implications of these results include the possibility of using personality testing for screening candidates for this treatment modality and the potential desirability of introducing a problem-solving skills training component to therapeutic foster care. Directions for further research were offered. In loving dedication to my husband, Martin Gregory, and our daughter, Jessica Nicole. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS In hindsight, I feel like my doctorate has been a lifetime in the making. I wish it were possible to acknowledge all of the people along the way who had an impact on my life. This dissertation has been the product of the support, encouragement, and guidance of some very special people. I would like to begin expressing my gratitude by thanking the faculty who served as my committee: Dr. Bill Hinds, Dr. John Schneider, Dr. Dick Johnson, and Dr. James Snoddy. Without their patience and guidance, this research endeavor would not have been possible. In particular, I would like to thank Dr. Bill Hinds, my committee chair, for his continuous encouragement, support, and direction in developing the study and shaping the dissertation. His faith in my work provided an anchor for me throughout the process. In addition, I feel indebted to Dr. Linda Forrest, who gave me emotional support in persevering in the program and inspired me with a new framework for looking at life. I would like to offer a special thanks to Dr. Joel Kelley, who not only provided me with a research vi instrument but also gave me emotional support in weathering the dissertation process, as well as Dr. Bonnie Fons, who graciously shared her knowledge and time. A special recognition needs to be given to Alice Kalush, Sue Cooley, Douglas Miller, and Richard Brown, who lent their professional expertise to this project. Any acknowledgment would not be complete without expressing heartfelt thanks to my parents, Virginia and Charles Crickon, for providing me with the love of reading and the value of education that brought me this far. It would also like to express my gratitude to my mother and Kym for providing my daughter with mother-substitutes during the many hours I was wrapped up in this dissertation process, and to Judy, Anne, and those relatives and friends who offered support and encouragement along the way. Finally, and most importantly, I would like to express my deepest love and appreciation to my husband, Marty, and our daughter, Jessie, who experienced many sacrifices so that I could pursue this doctorate. To Marty: Thank you for sharing my life--the emotional upheavals, the arduous tasks, and all the effort, none of which could ever culminate in a finding more significant than you. To Jessie: I hope this effort in some way has a positive influence on your life, the way you have given meaning to mine. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF GRAPHS . . . . . . . . . . . Chapter I. INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY . . Background . . . . Need . . . . . . . . Purpose . . . . . . . Research Questions . Theory . . . . . . . . . Summary and Overview . . II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE Review of the Literature on education . . . . . . . . General Skills Training . Specific Skills Training Review of the Literature on Solving . . . . . . . . . Personal Paradigm Shift . Review of the Literature on Foster Care . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . III. METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . Sample . . . . . . . . . . Population . . . . . . . Sample-Selection Criteria Description of the Sample Procedures . . . . . . . . Instrumentation . . . . . . The Millon Adolescent Person Inventory . . . . . . . viii Therapeutic < o o o o o 0 Po. 0 o o o o Page xi xii UitOmmUIH H 17 20 23 26 38 43 47 49 49 49 50 51 52 54 54 The Personal Change Questionnaire Survey . . Design . . . Data Analysis Summary . . . IV. DATA ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Descriptive Survey Results . . . . . Results Regarding Personality Profile of Adolescents in Therapeutic Foster Care . . . . . . . . . . . . Results Regarding Awareness of Elements Necessary for Problem Solving . . . . . . . . . . . . Results Regarding Possibility of a Correlation Between Personality Profiles and Length of Time in Therapeutic Foster Care . . . . . Results Regarding Possibility of a Correlation Between Length of Time in Therapeutic Foster Care and Awareness of Problem-Solving Elements . . . . . . . . . . . Results Regarding the Possibility of a Correlation Between Personality Profiles and Awareness of Problem- Solving Elements . . . . . . . . . summary 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Descriptive Data . . . . . . . . . Personality Profile . . . . . Perceived Problem-Solving Skills Correlation of Time in Therapeutic Foster Care With Personality Profiles . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correlation of Time in Therapeutic Foster Care With Perceived Problem- Solving Abilities . . . . . . . . . Correlation of Personality Profiles With Perceived Problem-Solving Abilities . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . Review of the Study . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions Regarding Demographic Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix Page 67 73 74 75 76 77 78 78 79 104 111 111 113 114 114 115 115 116 116 116 118 118 119 Page Conclusions Regarding Personality Prefile O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 120 Conclusions Regarding Self-Rating of Problem-Solving Skills . . . . . . . 127 Conclusions Regarding the Relationship Between Length of Time in Thera- peutic Foster Care and Personality . . 129 Conclusions Regarding the Relationship Between Length of Time in Therapeutic Foster Care and Perceived Problem- Solving Abilities . . . . . . . . . . 133 Conclusions Regarding the Relationship Between Personality and Perceived Problem-Solving Abilities . . . . . . . 135 Implications of the Results for Theory and for Therapeutic Foster Care Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137 Implications for Future Research . . . . 140 Exploratory Research . . . . . . . . . 140 Longitudinal Research . . . . . . . . . 140 Outcome Research . . . . . . . . . . . 141 Limitations of Observational Studies. . 142 APPENDICES A. THE MILLON ADOLESCENT PERSONALITY INVENTORY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 1 4 4 B. THE PERSONAL CHANGE QUESTIONNAIRE . . . . . 147 C. THE SURVEY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153 Table LIST OF TABLES Perceived Problem-Solving Abilities . Correlations of Length of Time in Thera- peutic Foster Care With MAPI Scales . Correlation of Length of Time in Thera- peutic Foster Care With the Personal Change Questionnaire . . . . . Correlation of Age of Adolescent With the Personal Change Questionnaire . xi 0 Page 109 111 112 113 Graph 1. lo. 11. 12. 13. 14. Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of Scale . . Comparison of LIST OF GRAPHS Results Results Results Results Results Results Results Results Results Results Esteem Scale . . . Comparison of Results scale 0 O O O I 0 Comparison of Results Acceptance Scale . Comparison of Results Scale . . . . . . Comparison of Results ance Scale . . . . xii on the Introversion on the Inhibited on the Cooperative on the Sociable Scale on the Confident on the Forceful on the Respectful on the Sensitive on the Self-Concept on the Personal on the Body Comfort on the Sexual on the Peer Security on the Social Toler- Page 81 83 84 86 88 90 91 93 95 96 97 98 100 101 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. Comparison of Results Scale . . . . . . Comparison of Results Confidence Scale . Comparison of Results Control Scale . . Comparison of Results formity Scale . . Comparison of Results Achievement Scale Comparison of Results Consistency Scale xiii on the Family Rapport on the Academic on the Impulse on the Social Con- on the Scholastic on the Attendance Page 102 103 105 106 107 108 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY This chapter begins with a brief historical overview of the evolution of therapeutic foster care, culminating with the legislation that has defined the focus of this type of treatment program. Following this explanation is a description of the need as well as its purpose. Because this is an observational analysis rather than an experimental design, the focus for this study is provided through five research questions that attempt to define and describe the therapeutic foster care population. Additionally, a brief overview is provided of the three major theories underlying this study: psychoeducation, problem. solving, and ‘therapeutic foster care as a treatment modality. Finally, a summary concludes this chapter. W From their inception and throughout their history, foster care placements have fluctuated between a custodial and a treatment orientation. The concept of foster care dates back to the Elizabethan Poor Law of 1601, when the colonies stipulated the' local care of the needy. Specifically, the law allowed for the apprenticeship of needy children to promote training aimed at being self- supporting in adulthood. This benevolent intention was translated pragmatically into the "binding out" or indenturing of orphaned, neglected, or destitute children, a practice that originated in England (Cox & Cox, 1985). In addition to indenturing, some communities also used vendue, which was the auctioning off of children to individuals who claimed they would care for the children for the lowest price (Cox & Cox, 1985). Thus, initially, this arrange— ment was of a custodial nature, with the children provid- ing a source of labor to the families in whose homes they were placed. Although the binding out of children provided support for the majority of children in need, by the mid- to late eighteenth century, large cities had begun to maintain almshouses, a precursor to institutionalization of dependent children. Because the conditions in these houses were often deplorable, resulting in a mortality rate in the 1800s of 80% to 90%, philanthropic and religious groups began sponsoring private orphanage asylums (Cox & Cox, 1985). By the 18305, these private childcare institutions had replaced the indenture system. In the years prior to the Civil War, a new type of childcare institution began to emerged. These facilities were called reformatories and were created for the placement of juvenile offenders. Rehabilitation rather than custodial care was emphasized. This laid the foundation for the treatment orientation of present-day childcare institutions. The plan at that time was to release the children after one or two years of treatment to be apprenticed to rural families. State Boards of Charity emerged at that time to supervise these childcare arrangements (Cox & Cox, 1985) . However, this treatment orientation was short-lived. Following the Civil War, an immense number of children were orphaned and displaced, resulting in a shift once again to merely custodial care. However, the social reform movement of the 19005 precipitated a change from relief to prevention. Charles Loring Brace founded the Children’s Aid Society, which advocated the use of foster homes, promoted the "science" of child placement, and emphasized attention to the needs of the child (Cox & Cox, 1985). As part of the reform movement, legislation was passed that outlawed the placement of children in almshouses. Private charity organizations flourished, and denominational foster care homes emerged. In 1909, President Theodore Roosevelt convened the first White House Conference on Children, which encouraged the shift from institutions to foster home placements. By 1920, the Child Welfare League of America had been formed to supervise the standards of childcare. Although the Social Security Act of 1935 established child welfare services, federal money was not available until 1947. The government actually did not grant any significant funding for foster care until 1961. This was immediately followed in 1962 by a change in the definition of child welfare services to include prevention and rehabilitation. However, this shift to a treatment orientation was short-lived. In 1967, the emphasis reverted to tangible, custodial services rather than counseling. The literature offers no explanation for this change in orientation (Cox & Cox, 1985). Foster care as a treatment tool resurfaced in 1971 with the publication of Foster Care of Children: Nurture a ea t, published by the U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. In 1975, the Child Welfare League of America continued this orientation in its Standards for 0 Se ' es. Under "Objective and Purpose," it stated: The ultimate objectives of foster family services should be the promotion of healthy personality development of the child, and the amelioration of problems that are personally or socially destructive. . . . Foster family care should provide, for the child whose own parents cannot do so, experiences and conditions that promote normal maturation (care), prevent further injury to the child (protection), and correct specific problems that interfere with healthy personality development (treatment). This clear statement of the treatment intent of foster care was followed in 1975 by the creation of Title XX, which authorized the following (Cox & Cox, 1985): a. community-based prevention and rehabilitation services: b. prevention and reduction of inappropriate insti- tutional care; c. referral to institutional care when appropriate. Elements (a) and (b) of Title XX form the foundation for the emergence of therapeutic foster care, a community- based prevention/rehabilitation program aimed at minimiz- ing inappropriate institutional care. Beginning in the late 19705, therapeutic foster' care 'treatment. programs started to appear in the literature. Need The :most recent. national statistics indicate ‘that approximately 396,000 children are in foster care homes (Cox & Cox, 1985). The proportion of adolescents within this population has doubled in the past few years from 24% to 49% (Timberlake & Verdieck, 1987). Timberlake and Verdieck (1987) offered explanations for the phenomenon of a higher representation of adoles- cents in foster care: 1. A larger number of children enter foster care at an early age and remain through adolescence. 2. A decrease in external family supports and social-control influences make out-of-home placements necessary for this age group. 3. An increased willingness appears to exist for parents to turn "problem" adolescents over to social service agencies. 4. Delinquent adolescents are being diverted into the child care system. This last point is part of a more basic trend of deinstitutionalization that has emerged in the mental health field in recent years. Within the past decade, the mandate to locate the least restrictive setting has resulted in a diversion of adolescents from correctional and psychiatric facilities into newly created therapeutic foster homes (Hawkins, Meadowcraft, Trout, & Imster, 1985). These diverted adolescents are prime candidates for a treatment-oriented foster care program. The therapeutic foster care programs that began to emerge in the late 19705 revolve specifically around a treatment component for foster care. The literature makes reference to a 75% to 82% success rate, which is neither defined nor quantified (Webb, 1988). Specific delineation of the characteristics of this population or definitive criteria for appropriate referrals to these programs have been lacking in the literature. Host descriptive statements merely note that these children have "behavioral or emotional problems." The question then becomes which children constitute appropriate referrals to therapeutic foster care, i.e., who is supposedly being successfully served by these programs? Profiles of this population are lacking. Identifying who is being served by therapeutic foster care programs would provide a foundation for later research into the efficacy of this treatment strategy and would ensure the appropriateness of referrals based on the evaluation of who benefits from this form of intervention. Quality assurance rests on the ability to evaluate program effectiveness. However, quality assurance cannot be ascertained when the population is ill-defined, leaving the targeted behaviors for change not clearly enumerated. Evaluation of a particular program’s effectiveness and justification for the choice and high expense of therapeutic foster care as a treatment option necessitate specificity of the characteristics of those children benefiting from this modality. The literature regarding therapeutic foster care describes each programs' functioning without performing an analysis of the population itself. An observational analysis of adolescents currently in therapeutic foster care could reveal some distinguishing patterns and could differentially identify factors that correlate with successful treatment and subsequent placement. ose The primary purpose of this study was to ascertain certain characteristics of the adolescents served by therapeutic foster' care jprograms. To accomplish this task, selected therapeutic foster care programs were examined with the intention of identifying characteristics of the adolescents in placements. This examination included assessing the personality profile of the children as well as examining the effect of treatment by measuring their performance on a personal change questionnaire. Finally, attempts were made to identify factors that correlated with length of stay in placement. Resear ue 1on5 1. Is there a personality profile of adolescents in therapeutic foster care? 2. Are adolescents in therapeutic foster care aware of elements necessary for personal change or problem solving? 3. What is the correlation between personality pro- files and length of time in therapeutic foster care? 4. What is the correlation between length of time in therapeutic foster care and awareness of personal change or problem-solving elements? 5. What is the correlation between personality pro- files and awareness of personal change or problem-solving elements? Theory This section contains an overview of the three major theories that underlie this study: psychoeducation, problem solving, and therapeutic foster care. The section begins with a macroscopic theoretical perspective by outlining the broad field of psychoeducation. This discourse narrows to discuss a specific psychoeducational intervention model, the problem-solving approach, which can be applied in various treatment settings. Finally, the theory behind therapeutic foster care is explored to help in understanding the treatment program selected for this study. Psychoeducation entered the mental health scene 35 years ago, emerging from the theoretical coupling of the fields of education and psychology (Sanford, 1955). Withstanding the tests of time and professional scrutiny, psychoeducation has gained legitimacy as an assessment and intervention strategy (Authier, Gustafson, Guerney, & Kasdorf, 1975; Goss, 1964; Guerney, Guerney & Stollak, 1971/1972: Larson, 1984: Masher & Sprinthall, 1971: Miller, 1969). 10 The crux of this new model has been to provide a teaching format for the delivery of human services. In particular, the practitioner and client focus on the development of personal and interpersonal skills and attitudes that the client could apply to resolve present and future psychological problems (Guerney et al., 1970). Inherent in the psychoeducational approach are three basic assumptions (Larson, 1984): l. The practitioner is a teacher of the client. Therefore, therapeutic intervention is seen as a learning process (Guerney et al., 1970, 1972). 2. The client’s problems are viewed as a skill deficit or lack of competency that can be overcome with training. 3. The client is viewed as an active, self- responsible, and self-directing person, even in the treatment setting. Within the context of this new psychoeducational model, one of the areas of research has been the facilitation of competency in personal problem solving and decision making (Parker, 1987). The major advantage in learning a problem-solving process in treatment is that the principles can be readily transferred from one situation or content area to another (Hepworth & Larsen, 1990). A client's acquisition of these skills would not only remedy the immediate problem but would also serve a 11 preventive function by enhancing the client’s future coping capacity. Problem-solving theory revolves around three core terms that need to be operationalized at this point: "problem," "problem-solving" behavior, and "solution." The term "problem" refers to a specific situation or set of related situations to which a person must respond in order to function effectively in his/her environment. Accordingly, a situation would be deemed problematic if no effective response alternative was immediately available to the individual confronted with the situation (Davis, 1966: Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Skinner, 1953). Included in this definition are all those situations that, due to novelty, complexity, ambiguity, or conflicting stimulus demands, fail to (elicit "automatic" effective actions, thus requiring problem-solving behavior. "Problem solving" is defined as a behavioral process, whether overt or cognitive in nature, which (a) makes available a variety of potentially effective response alternatives for dealing with the problematic situation and (b) increases the probability of selecting the most effective response from among these various alternatives (D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971). Thus, this definition includes both the generation of alternative responses as well as decision-making or choice behavior. 12 Finally, a "solution" or effective response refers to the response or pattern of responses that alters the situation so that it is no longer problematic to the individual and at the same time maximizes other positive consequences and minimizes other negative ones (D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971). Using this "problem--problem solving--solution" para- digm, individuals could be perceived as differing in their ability to problem-solve. This differential functioning could be viewed as existing on a continuum. On one end are those individuals deemed to be effective in their decision-making capacity. Socrates observed that competent individuals are "those who manage well the circumstances which they encounter daily, and who possess a judgment which is accurate in meeting occasions as they arise and rarely miss the expedient course of action." Juxtapositioned to these individuals are those who appear unable to appropriately resolve certain situational problems in their lives and who, accordingly, bear the labels of "abnormal," "deviant," or "emotionally disturbed." These are frequently the labels worn by the adolescents placed in the therapeutic foster care treat- ment programs examined in this study. Theoretically, the core of these therapeutic foster care treatment programs is the use of specially selected and trained foster parents, who act as paraprofessionals 13 and implement treatment programs designed by agency staff for adolescents or children within the family’s context (Hawkins et al., 1985). The main component of this strategy is the "professional" parent who functions as the main "agent of treatment" rather than merely as a custodial caregiver. Other specific features, according to Hawkins et al. (1985), include: 1. Supervisory staff, who provide a range of services to the foster parents and to the adolescents and their families. 2. A recruitment and training program to obtain highly qualified foster parents. 3. An emphasis on professionalism of the parenting role as indicated by a salary for the foster parent commensurate with that of a beginning child care worker, rather than merely being provided room and board reim- bursement. 4. A high degree of accountability of the supervisory staff, the foster parents, and the adoles- cents. 5. Extensive foster parent support services, including 24-hour on-call emergency services; liaison services with the school, courts, and other community agencies: respite care: and intensive supervision (often weekly rather than the monthly or quarterly supervision provided to regular foster care families). 14 6. An individualized point system to motivate the adolescent's behavior in the foster home and to direct treatment goals. 7. Foster parent and adolescent evaluations to provide feedback. 8. Treatment services to the adolescent's biological family. The theoretical advantages of this treatment model have been well-documented (Hawkins et al., 1985; Hazel, 1982; Lanier & Coffey, 1981: Larson, Allison, & Johnson, 1978: Rubenstein, Armentrout, Levin, & Harold, 1978). A brief summary is as follows: First, the disruption to the adolescent’s life is minimized in that opportunity to engage in normal activities is facilitated by a family setting. Second, a greater proportion of the cost of the placement is invested directly into the treatment of the child rather than into buildings, maintenance staff, equipment, or other typical residential expenses. Third, because an actual facility does not need to be maintained, there is less pressure either to retain adolescents after treatment goals have been achieved or to accept inappropriate referrals. Fourth, treatment is highly individualized because of the placement of only one or two children per home. 15 Fifth, the behaviors targeted for change are highly relevant to the tasks of daily living because of the context of that treatment. Thus, these new behaviors are more likely to transfer to the adolescent's own home due to the similarity of settings. Sixth, positive incidental learning occurs as a: result of exposure to a relatively healthy family. Finally, a broad range of adolescent clients can be served by this program model due to the individualized treatment and separate placement opportunities. Thus, theoretically speaking, therapeutic foster care programs provide the opportunity to target the problem- solving skills of the adolescents in care and alter their "deviant" labeling. One of the goals of this study was to investigate whether those problem-solving skills were affected by the therapeutic foster care programs examined. Summary and ngryiew This chapter contained an overview of the evolution of the therapeutic foster care programs that provides the treatment context for this study. Additionally, the need and purpose of this study were described, along with five research questions designed to define and describe the therapeutic foster care population being examined. The chapter ended with a brief presentation of the three 16 theories underlying this study: psychoeducation, problem solving, and therapeutic foster care. In Chapter II, the relevant literature is reviewed for the following three areas: psychoeducational theory, problem-solving theory, and therapeutic foster care. The research procedures and design are presented in Chapter III, along with a discussion of the construction and validity of the Millon Adolescent Personality Inventory and the Personal Change Questionnaire. The analysis of the results is described in Chapter IV, while conclusions and recommendations for future research are presented in Chapter V. CHAPTER I I REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE This chapter contains a literature review of the three major theories used in this study. Moving from the macroscopic to the microscopic perspective, the chapter begins with a review of the literature on psychoeducational theory. This is followed by a literature review on a type of psychoeducational model, problem-solving theory (which includes the theory behind the Personal Paradigm Shift, a specific problem-solving approach contributing to this study). Finally, the chapter ends by focusing on a literature review of therapeutic foster care, a specific treatment concept in which problem-solving theory can be applied. v' h te t s educ ' The historical development of the psychoeducational approach can be traced through the cumulative beliefs of various theorists and researchers over the past century. As far back as Watson (1916), researchers believed that human attitudes, interests, values and behavior could be extremely malleable with proper environmental 17 18 manipulation. In accordance with this philosophy, psychoeducation views much of human behavior as the result of learning and experience rather than instinct or genetic predisposition. Later, Skinner (1938, 1953) provided direction for learning-oriented therapists when he developed the concept of operant conditioning. Skinner strengthened the basis for the psychoeducational approach with his focus on targeting specific teaching objectives regarding behaviors and by developing specific teaching programs for accomplishing those goals. Using this learning-based approach, Wblpe (1958) targeted his interventions toward the "psychologically distressed." An almost simultaneous factor at this time for the evolving psychoeducational approach was Albee’s (1959) warning to the profession that a need-supply gap was imminent. Through his writings, he urged a shift from the traditional counseling model, in order to have the supply of counseling services meet the projected demand that he anticipated. This push for a new model was occurring as decades of research had allegedly failed to provide strong, convincing proof that traditional therapy was effective and.‘efficient (Eysenck, 1952: Levitt, 1957, 1963). Psychoeducation was one of the nontraditional approaches that gained strength with these disputed research findings. 19 At this same time, the emergence of the importance of prevention rather than remediation resulted in a shift to a more instructional model of intervention. This shift entailed a change in emphasis with goals and interventive style. Rogers (1951) ushered in a portion of this shift with his focused attention on goals and responsibility, with an emphasis on client determination. Specific impetus for the cognitive-emotional-behaviora1 style that characterizes psychoeducation was contributed by Ellis (1963), who stressed teaching cognitive-emotional skills to people for their use in facilitating their own personal and interpersonal goals. Following these initial historical roots, the psychoeducational approach began to gain its own identity when theorists like Patterson and Carkhuff (1969) urged psychological practitioners to consider educational training as a therapeutic modality. Patterson and Carkhuff stated that perhaps therapy was not necessary. Instead they supported direct training or education of individuals in the "conditions of good human relations-- not only normal people and children but the emotionally disturbed as well" (p. ix). As part of this emergence of its own identity, psychoeducation was defined and differentiated from traditional therapy by theorists like Authier, Gustafson, Guerney and Kasdorf (1975). 20 Traditional therapy was perceived as following an illness model: "abnormality--diagnosis--prescription--therapy-- cure" (Authier et al., 1975). The psychoeducational model has a pattern of "client dissatisfaction--goa1 setting-- skill teaching--satisfaction or goal achievement." The client is perceived as a pupil who is taught not only symbolic and cognitive processes of change but also instruction in overt behavior. Within the psychoeducational approach are two types of programs: one that focuses on general skills training and the other that emphasizes specific skills“ The following provides examples in the literature of the two models. k' s 'n'n General skills training programs stress counseling skills, communicationt skills, interpersonal skills, and relationship skills. Carkhuff (1971) and Ivey (1971) developed two of the most popular general skills training programs. Carkhuff’s program has been used to teach the interpersonal skills of empathy, respect, concreteness, genuineness, self-disclosures, confrontation, and immediacy to various groups of psychiatric inpatients (Pierce & Drasgow, 1969: Vitalo, 1971), to parents of emotionally disturbed children (Carkhuff & Bierman, 1970), 21 to college students (Berenson, Carkhuff & Myrus, 1966), to Head Start teachers (Bierman, Carkhuff & Santilli, 1969), to groups consisting of teachers and parents of different races (Carkhuff & Banks, 1969), and others. Ivey's (1971) program emphasizes less global communication skills, such as attending behaviors, open- invitation-to-talk, paraphrasing, minimal-encouragement- to-talk, and reflection-of-feeling. Ivey's program has also been used with various populations, including psychiatric inpatients (Donk, 1971; Ivey, 1973; Orlando, 1974) and. psychiatric outpatients (Galassi, Galassi, & Litz, 1974: Gormally, Hill, Otis & Rainey, 1975). Both Ivey's and Carkhuff’s programs involve teaching a variety of communication skills by explicit instruction and feedback in graded practice. The therapist's role is, thus, conceptualized as that of a teacher who uses behavioral shaping with emphasis on verbal reinforcement. Additionally, role playing in dyads or triads is frequently used, along with audio and/or video feedback. In addition to the Carkhuff and Ivey programs, other models have been developed to teach general skills competency. These include the Step Group Therapy model, the Conj ugal Relationship Enhancement Program, and the Parent Effectiveness Training Program, which are briefly described in the following paragraphs. 22 The Step Group Therapy program by Authier and Fix (1977) is a blend of group therapy with behavior therapy using the psychoeducational format. The program consists of teaching nine communication skills in a series of three steps. Patients are promoted from one group to the next by demonstrating competency in that particular set of skills emphasized at each step. The program uses written definitions of skills, model tapes, didactic instruction, and a counselor who acts as a teacher, using the above modalities along with successive approximation reinforcements to encourage the development of skills. The Conjugal Relationship Enhancement Program (Ely, Guerney, & Stover, 1973) explicitly teaches the communication skills inherent in the roles of "speaker" and "listener" in marriage counseling situations. This model has been adapted and used by Collins (1971) and Rappaport (1971). Another marital counseling psychoeduca- tional program by Weiss, Hops and Patterson (1973) teaches the more specific skills of contracting and negotiating in relationships. Gordon’s (1973) Parent Effectiveness Training teaches both basic communications skills and specific parenting skills. A similar parenting program has been developed by Patterson and Gullian (1968). 23 MW Specific skills training programs are numerous and address diverse populations. Probably the most widely known is the Structured Learning Therapy developed by Goldstein (1973). This program is unique in that it was specifically designed to fill the void mentioned earlier between public demand for psychological services and the number of psychological practitioners available to meet the need. Furthermore, the program was intentionally designed to address the lifestyles, need, and environmental realities of the lower socioeconomic class because traditional methods of treatment have not always been adequate or appropriate for this population. Structured Learning Therapy focuses on the use of modeling, role playing, and social reinforcement to enhance patient autonomy, assertiveness, internal controls, role-taking abilities, sense of mastery in social interactions, accuracy of affective perception and communication, tolerance for frustration and ambiguity, and other useful behaviors in which the person may be deficient. Structured Learning Therapy has been successfully used with various psychiatric populations (Gutride, Goldstein, & Hunter, 1973, to increase social behavior of psychiatric inpatients: Goldstein et al., 1973, to teach independence to outpatient and inpatient populations). 24 Gutride, Goldstein, and Hunter (1974) contributed one of the more recent additions to the Structured Learning Therapy, i.e., the development of a transfer of training phase. This phase directly teaches trainees skills that will help them cope with stress when they return to their home environments. Thus, the teaching of coping skills has become an important adjunct to the psychoeducational model. In addition to the Structured Learning Therapy program, other specific skills training models have been directed toward various populations. These can be briefly enumerated to reflect the breadth of applicability of this type of program, based on research in the literature: job hunting (Hunt & Azrin, 1973); interpersonal communication skills (Rathus, 1973); developmental tasks of children and adolescents (Gum, Tamminen, & Smaby, 1973; MacMullen, 1973): sexual satisfaction (Masters & Johnson, 1970); irrational thoughts (Ellis, 1961, 1963): controlling problem behavior, problem solving, and decision making (Krumboltz & Thoresen, 1969; Mahoney, 1974); coping with frustration (Giebink, Stover, & Fahl, 1968): handling aggressive impulses (Gittelman, 1965); alleviating depression (Lewinsohn, Weinstein, & Shaw, 1969: Idbert & Lewinsohn, 1973): learning constructive marital conflict resolution (Bach & Wyden, 1969): "healthy talk" by 25 schizophrenics (Meichenbaum, 1969): and. problem-solving skills training programs (D'Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971). In addition to this breadth of usage and most relevant for the purposes of this study, the psychoeducational approach has been repeatedly used to alter the learning behavior of disturbed/delinquent children and adolescents (Beck & McDonnell, 1982: Brown, 1980: Brown & Quay, 1978: Glick & Goldstein, 1987: Konstantareas & Homatidis, 1984). Two research examples of using the psychoeducational model with delinquents are Minuchin, Chamberlain, and Graubard (1967) and Carpenter and Sugrue (1984). Minuchin et al. (1967) used a psychoeducatonal approach with disturbed/delinquent children by training them to focus attention, to organize and obtain information through communication, and to search for solutions to conflicts when interacting with teachers. Improvements were significant in attention, style of communication, and cognition. Carpenter' and Sugrue (1984) used the psychoeducational model with a heterogeneous delinquent treatment program. They combined affective education and social skills training in an outpatient setting. They found that delinquents with immature personalities or neurotic features appeared to benefit more from this approach than those with characterological disorders. 26 In summary, as can be noted from the above references, the literature is replete with studies attempting to identify what characteristics or populations are most amenable to the psychoeducational approach. The research results have indicated that the psychoeducational model has been used successfully in diverse contexts with various populations, including emotionally disturbed or delinquent adolescents. One of the specific skills training approaches within the field of psychoeducation is problem solving. v' o ' u o The clinical application of problem-solving training had its beginnings in the late 19605 and early 19705 as part of the trend toward clinical interventions that focused on the facilitation of social competence. Developmentally speaking, D'Zurilla and Goldfried (1971) provided the theoretical underpinnings of problem-solving theory. Their research, which spanned more than 15 years, resulted in the following two beliefs: 1. The first belief is that ineffective coping with problematic situations, along with its personal and social consequences, may‘ be the necessary and sufficient condition for an emotional or behavioral disorder requiring psychological treatment. 27 2. The second belief is that problem solving may be most efficiently developed by training individuals in general procedures or skills. These two findings have relevance for the adolescents in this study. Since the therapeutic foster care adolescents have been identified as having emotional/ behavioral disorders requiring treatment, they fit within the first belief system of D’Zurilla and Goldfried. Accordingly, the possibility of being able to remedy their deficient problem solving through skills training could provide a direction for future experimental research into the adolescent population and therapeutic foster care. In line with the theoretical conceptualizations of D’Zurilla and Goldfried (1971), problem solving can be viewed as a cognitive-affective-behavioral process, culminating in the discovery of a solution to a problem. D'Zurilla and Goldfried proposed a prescriptive or normative model of social problem solving that focused on how individuals should solve problems in order to maximize their effectiveness (as opposed to a descriptive model that merely describes how individuals typically solve problems). The D’Zurilla and Goldfried model was based on a melding of relevant problem-solving theory and research literature from the fields of experimental psychology, education and industry. The final D'Zurilla model 28 consisted of the following five components: problem orientation, problem definition and formulation, generation of alternative solutions, decision making, and solution. implementation and 'verification. Individually, each component serves a definite purpose or function in the problem-solving process. Combined, the five components are expected to maximize the probability of ascertaining the most effective solution when appropriately applied to a particular problem. Following the lead of D'Zurilla and Goldfried (1971), Spivack, Platt, and. Shure (1976) provided support for exploring the problem-solving capability of clients. In their research, they compared a group of "deviant" individuals with a group of "normal" individuals. Their results indicated that the deviant group demonstrated inferior skills in problem solving compared to the normal group with respect to three major areas: 1. The deviant group generated fewer possible solutions to hypothetical problem situations. 2. The deviant group suggested solutions that were often antisocial in nature. 3. The deviant group had very inaccurate expecta- tions about probable consequences of alternative solu- tions. 29 Furthermore, Spivak et al.'s (1976) results indicated that promising results could be obtained when deviant subjects received systematic training in problem solving. Substantial research can be found in the literature on the use of problem-solving skills training with adolescents, as can be noted by the references in the following paragraphs. Kazdin, Esveldt-Dawson, French, & Unis (1987) focused on problem-solving skills training by comparing three conditions: cognitive-behavioral problem-solving skills training to nondirective relationship therapy and a control condition for antisocial children. Fifty-six children between the ages of 7 and 13 were randomly assigned to one of four therapists and the three conditions. The children were hospitalized in a psychiatric facility during the period of treatment and discharged thereafter. The problem-solving training condition led to significantly less externalizing and aggressive behaviors and fewer behavioral problems at home and at school and more prosocial behaviors than the other two conditions. These effects were evident immediately after treatment and at a 1-year follow—up. Tisdelle and St. Lawrence (1988) examined the effectiveness of social problem-solving skills training with conduct-disordered in-patient adolescents. Training criteria were socially validated with nonhospitalized 30 adolescents. Three baseline assessment sessions were conducted before the intervention, and further assessments were administered after each training session. Only when the subjects attained the skill level exhibited by the normal adolescents in the comparison group did the training progress to the next phase. Five problem-solving component skills were taught to the adolescents: problem identification, goal definition, generation of alterna- tives, comparison of consequences, and selection of the best alternative. Verbal problem solving improved, generalized to unfamiliar problem situations, and was maintained at a l-month follow-up assessment. Christoff et a1. (1985) evaluated the effectiveness of a problem-solving training program for adolescents. The subjects were four adolescents ranging in age from 12 to 14 years who were referred to the training program because they were socially withdrawn and lacking in social skills. The results showed a significant increase in both problem-solving skills and specific social interaction skills. All improvements were maintained at a 5-month follow-up assessment. Sarason and Sarason (1981) used this approach to enhance problem-solving cognitions and effective coping behavior in problematic situations relevant to adolescents. The successful results were maintained at a 31 1-year follow-up. In addition, objective measures within the school setting were also apparent, including lower rates of tardiness, fewer absences, and fewer referrals to school personnel for problem behavior. Other problem-solving skills training programs have been developed and evaluated with successful results. Some programs have had the benefit of replicated research. Jacobson (1977) fine-tuned his research using a treatment and control group by adding a nonspecific therapy group (Jacobson, 1978). The following summary of his 1977 and 1978 research highlights his findings. Jacobson (1977) developed a behavior marital therapy program using problem-solving skills training. The program was initially evaluated in an outcome study involving 10 distressed marital couples, who were randomly assigned to either a treatment group (involving 10 sessions) or a waiting-list control group. The results indicated that the couples in treatment improved significantly more than the waiting-list control group on both problem-solving behavior and marital adjustment measures. At a l-year follow-up, the treatment gains were maintained. In addition, frequency of problematic behaviors in the home was significantly affected by the specific problem-solving treatment procedures. Jacobson (1978) conducted a second study to replicate the above findings, allowing for the addition of a 32 nonspecific therapy group to control for nonspecific factors such as attention and expectation of benefit. The nonspecific group included discussions of the relationship problems but contained no specific problem-solving skills training. The results confirmed the findings of the first study. In addition, the results showed that the problem- solving treatment group demonstrated significantly more improvement than the nonspecific treatment group on measures of problem-solving behavior and marital adjustments. These gains were maintained at a 6-month follow-up assessment. Robin (1979, 1980) also fine-tuned his initial research. The problem-solving program he designed for parent-adolescent dyads experiencing conflict has been evaluated in three clinical outcome studies: one by Robin, Kent, O’Leary, Foster, and Prinz (1977), one by Robin (1981), and one by Foster, Prinz, and O'Leary (1983). In the first study, Robin et a1. (1977) randomly assigned 22 mother-adolescent dyads to a problem-solving training group or a waiting-list control group. The results demonstrated dramatic improvement in problem- solving behavior in the problem-solving group relative to the control group at the post-treatment assessment. However, no definitive evidence of improvement in conflict resolution behavior was noted at home. 33 In the second study, Foster et a1. (1983) randomly assigned 28 families to a waiting-list control group, a problem-solving group with generalization programming, or a problem-solving group without generalization training. The results indicated that both treatment groups improved significantly more than the control group on several measures of parent-adolescent conflict and communication behavior at home. These results were generally maintained at a 6- to 8-week follow-up assessment. The lack of significant differences between the two treatment groups was attributed to enhancements in the problem-solving training, such as inclusion of fathers in the program instead of mothers and adolescents only, an emphasis during training on the families' real problems instead of hypothetical ones, and the addition of two sessions. In the third study (Robin, 1981), problem solving was compared to a less structured "best alternative treatment" in an attempt to control for nonspecific factors such as expectations of benefit. The alternative treatment was short-term. family' therapy' conducted in an eclectic or family systems approach. Thirty-three families were randomly assigned to the two treatment groups or to a waiting-list control group. The results showed that both treatments produced significant improvement in conflict resolution and communication behavior at home. Only the problem-solving group demonstrated significant improvement 34 in problem-solving communication. These treatment effects were maintained at a 10-week follow-up. Furthermore, the parents in the problem-solving group rated their experience more favorably than did the family therapy group with regard to improvement in their relationship with their adolescents, improvement in their adolescents' behavior at home, improvement in problem-solving discussions at home, and the extent to which the program fulfilled their expectations. Research has also been done on the use of the problem-solving model for reducing stress and enhancing ineffective coping skills. Two studies regarding coping were by Moon and Eisler (1983) and Dixon, Heppner, Petersen, and Ronning (1979). Moon and Eisler (1983) compared the problem-solving group to a stress-inoculation and a social-skills training group when examining anger control issues. The results showed that stress inoculation significantly decreased anger-provoking cognitions but did not increase appropriate assertiveness. However, both the problem- solving training group and the social skills training group significantly reduced anger-provoking cognitions and increased assertive or socially appropriate behaviors. Dixon et a1. (1979) also used problem-solving skills training to enhance the generation of alternatives and 35 choice behavior with undergraduate college students. The subjects were randomly assigned to the problem-solving group, a pretest-posttest control group and a posttest- only control group. The problem-solving group showed significantly better problem-solving skills; the difference resulted primarily from less impulsivity in problem-solving situations. Heppner, Hibel, Neal, Weinstein, and Rabinowitz (1982) also later studied the effects of the problem- solving model with college students with problem-solving deficits. Subjects were randomly assigned to the problem- solving group or a delayed-treatment control group. Following training, the subjects in the problem-solving group, compared to the control group, reported fewer problems on the problem checklist, more problem-solving confidence, a greater tendency to approach rather than avoid problems, and a more favorable self-assessment of their problem-solving skills. These gains were maintained at a l-year follow-up evaluation. Similar results were obtained in research by Parnes and Noller (1973) conducted over a 2-year period with college freshmen. Problem solving has also been used in the treatment of depression. Nezu (1986) randomly assigned 26 clinically depressed subjects to one of three conditions: problem-solving therapy, problem-focused therapy, or a waiting-list control group. Statistical analyses and 36 clinical analyses indicated substantial reductions in depression in the problem-solving group. These results were maintained over a 6-month follow-up period. In addition, the improvement in depression was significantly greater in the problem-solving group than in the other two groups. This superiority was maintained at a 6-month follow-up evaluation. Furthermore, the results indicated that the problem-solving group increased significantly more than the other two groups in self-appraisal of problem-solving effectiveness and also in locus of control orientations from external to internal. Again, these improvements were also maintained at the 6-month follow- up assessment. Hussi‘an and Lawrence (1981) also used problem- solving skills training in the treatment of depression with geriatric patients in a nursing home. The problem- solving group showed significant improvement relative to the waiting-list control group and the social reinforcement training group. This superiority was present at a 2-week follow-up, although the differences were no longer significant at the 3-month follow-up assessment. Other researchers have also examined the use of problem-solving training with psychiatric patients (Bedell, Archer, & Marlowe, 1980: Coche, Cooper, & 37 Petermann, 1984: Coche & Flick, 1975; Edelstein, Couture, Cray, Dickens, & Lusebrink, 1980: Hansen, St. Lawrence, & Christoff, 1985: Siegel & Spivack, 1976a, 1976b). The problem-solving therapy approach also has been used successfully with weight-control problems (Black & Scherba, 1983: Black & Threlfall, 1986: Straw & Terre, 1983), alcoholism (Chaney, O’Leary, & Marlatt, 1978; Intagliatia, 1978), vocational indecision (Mendonca & Siess, 1976), academic underachievement (Richards & Perri, 1978), agoraphobia (Jannoun, Munby, Catalan, & Gelder, 1980), and with community problems (Briscoe, Hoffman & Bailey, 1975). Given the wealth of research on problem solving following the original findings of D'Zurilla and Goldfried (1971), this dissertation study included a specific problem-solving model, the Personal Paradigm Shift, when examining the adolescent population in therapeutic foster care. The Personal Paradigm Shift was developed by Hinds (1983) at Michigan State University with the intention of educating the client in therapy in a manner that would promote learning of the process of personal change. The paradigm approaches change as a problem-solving process with four phases that closely follow the final components of the D'Zurilla model: awareness of the problem, decision making as a process, selection of oppositional 38 alternatives to targeted behaviors, and the planning and implementation of necessary actions. so a1 ad' S i Since the Personal Paradigm Shift forms the foundation for one of the instruments used in this study (i.e., the Personal Change Questionnaire), an explanation of the paradigm stages is deemed appropriate at this point (Hinds, 1983). Stage 1--Awareness. This stage consists of seven steps aimed at creating self-understanding. The essence of this stage is that the individual confronts and examines the critical relationships between internal and external variables that contribute to the maintenance of personal problems. Step 1: instructs the individual to describe a personal problem which he/she wants to resolve. Step 2: asks the individual to set initial goals related to the problem chosen. (These goals are operationalized as behavioral objectives, aimed at assisting the individual in looking toward the future with optimism regarding possible resolution.) Step 3: asks the individual to identify the antecedent events to the problem. Step 4: has the individual identify the internal reactions, i.e. beliefs, feelings, mental images, and physiological reactions, that occur within the individual that accompany the antecedent events. Step 5: focuses on helping the individual see the sequential chain of behavioral actions that are taken by that individual when the problem situation occurs. 39 Step 6: has the individual identify the consequences that follow the actions addressed in Step 5. Step 7: asks the individual to describe what beliefs or needs must be abandoned before behavior can be changed and the problem resolved. (This final step is a unique feature that is not found in other behavior change or problem-solving models.) W This stage is aimed at having the individual use the information gained in the previous stage to examine his/her decision-making process. Decision making is seen as consisting of three steps: Step 1: constructing a balance sheet of perceived rewards and punishments entailed in solving the problem versus not changing. Step 2: making a cognitive and emotional commitment to change versus accepting the current situation as satisfactory. Step 3: confronting the resistance to change and identifying the fears associated with changing and resolving the problem. --0 s' 'o ' s. This stage attempts to help the individual develop a new set of internal reactions to foster a sense of self-control. The four steps in this stage are aimed at generating new beliefs, feelings, physiological reactions, and mental images to counter those identified in the Awareness Stage. The individual is asked to record the stressful conditions of the problem situation as part of the first step and then to think of new healthy internal reactions to oppose the existing ones as part of the second step. The third 40 step has the individual think about new actions to replace old problematic behaviors. The final step has the individual identify the new consequences that will follow the new actions. a -- n . The final stage consists of ten steps aimed at reducing the individual’s fear and increasing his/her motivation for change. This stage targets the individual's feelings of helplessness, hopelessness, and loss of control. Step 1: asks the individual to identify the initial goal he/she needs to achieve to maintain motivation. This goal must be realistic, specific, countable, and in behavioral terms. Step 2: focuses on recognizing and creating support systems for instituting change. Step 3: uses the concepts of observational learning and modeling in having the individual seek information and identify others who have achieved their goals and, thus, resolved their problems. Step 4: has the individual identify what appropriate reward he/she will provide him/herself following the accomplishment of the initial goal change. The individual must be able to use this positive reinforcement as a continued source of motivation throughout this process. Step 5: is unique to this model in that the individual is taught to organize a plan of attack for working on the personal problem. The individual is asked to clearly delineate the appropriate order of behaviors he/she will need to perform to achieve the desired behavior change and resolve the problem. Step 6: teaches the individual the meaning of feedback and how to develop a specific feedback system to maintain motivation. 41 Step 7: helps the individual develop a time frame for accomplishing goals and resolving problems. Step 8: assists the individual in developing back-up plans in the event that he/she encounters failure in first trying to resolve the problem. Step 9: addresses the issue of fear of success, i.e., the individual's unconscious attempt to undermine personal change and return to self-defeating behavior. Step 10: has the individual keep a daily or weekly log to record the specific gains made in achieving behavior change and resolving problems. This written record is intended to provide the individual with a support system to maintain motivation toward problem resolution. With the Personal Paradigm Shift, as with other psychoeducational problem-solving models, the ultimate goal is to teach the individual the process of personal change and to have the individual see him/herself as an active agent making behavioral choices that either facilitate or impede problem resolution. TwO research studies have been performed using the Personal Paradigm Shift. One study was by Jeney (1985), and the other by Parker (1987). In his dissertation, Jeney (1985) used the Personal Paradigm Shift to assess the pain perceptions, health behavior, and psychological disturbance of 51 chronic-pain patients. A multivariate analysis of variance on the subjects as a group was performed on the pain data and found to be nonsignificant for both the pretest-posttest comparison and the pretest follow-up comparison. The 42 author contended that the reasons for lack of significance included the small sample size, inappropriate dependent measures, the improvement evidenced in both groups, the brevity of the treatment period, and the subjects' rejection of the rehabilitation model. However, Parker (1987) in his dissertation used the Personal Paradigm Shift in assessing the stress, health behavior, and academic achievement of college students who had been identified as academic underachievers. Again the group, not the individual, was the unit of analysis regarding gain scores obtained from pretest-posttest and pretest follow-up comparisons. A two-way analysis of variance performed on the pretest follow-up comparison of quarterly grade point averages found a significant difference for those who had used the Personal Paradigm Shift. Parker stated that the absence of significance in other aspects of the design might have been attributable to the brief treatment period, the inappropriate dependent measures, the size of the treatment group, the evidence of improvement in both groups, and the developmental and personality factors that characterized the subjects involved in the study. In summary, the literature on problem-solving theory, including the research on the Personal Paradigm Shift, supported the use of problem-solving skills training with 43 various populations for application not only at a remedial level but also for prevention and/or enhancement. ;- —. .7 - ,'te -tur- ., - -,:--- .- e gee Because therapeutic foster care is a relatively new phenomenon, the literature, both descriptive and evaluative, is scant. Webb ( 1988) commented that, in 1983, 32 private agencies administered specialized therapeutic foster care programs in Michigan alone. Most of these programs had been developed since 1978. Webb indicated that evaluations have tended to be anecdotal, without research supporting the efficacy of therapeutic foster care as a treatment modality. In fact, she was able to find only one comparative study (Rubenstein, Armentrout, Levin, & Harold, 1978). In summation of her analysis of the literature on therapeutic foster care, Webb asserted the need for further evaluations to determine whether therapeutic foster care as a treatment modality can effectively influence a odearly defined population. In their comparative study, Rubenstein et a1. (1978) discussed the Parent-Therapist Program in Ontario, Canada. This program was designed for emotionally disturbed latency-age children (ages 6 to 12) as an alternative to institutionalization. An experimental design was used to evaluate the program. Twenty-seven children were admitted 44 into the Parent-Therapist Program, and 45 children were placed in residential treatment centers. The average length of time in placement was 18.7 months. The researchers used the Quay-Peterson Behavior Problem Checklist to ascertain the degree of behavioral disturbance. A three-way analysis of variance revealed that both groups showed improvement not related to the treatment modality to which they were assigned. However, the authors contended that the Parent-Therapist Program had a per diem rate of $24.05 to $30.16, compared to a residential treatment rate of $47.43 to $63.77. The authors, therefore, noted that therapeutic foster homes are more cost-effective than residential treatment. Three articles focused on the subsequent placement of children after discharge from therapeutic foster care as providing a positive assessment of the treatment modality (Bauer & Heinke, 1976: Hawkins, Meadowcraft, Trout, & Luster, 1985; Larson, Allison, & Johnson, 1978). Bauer and Heinke reviewed the therapeutic foster care program developed at the Fond du Lac Office of the Wisconsin Department of Health and Social Services. In its 8 years of operation, the program had placed 175 children, ranging in age from 2-1/2 to 17 years. No behavioral or psychological criteria for referral were indicated. Of the 42 children discharged, 29% returned to live with their birth parents, 23% were placed in adoption, 12% 45 entered independent living situations, 22% were placed in group homes or other treatment homes, and 7% entered institutions. From these outcome measures, the authors determined that "most of the youngsters who have participated in the program have made significant gains." Larson et a1. (1978) article discussed the Alberta Parent Counselors program in Canada. Eighty-seven children between the ages of 8 and 16 were placed in therapeutic foster homes. More than 50% had committed delinquencies; half were a grade or more behind in school. All demonstrated conduct problems, such as aggressiveness, withdrawal, social anxiety, depression, hyperactivity, or disobedience. The average placement was 8.7 months. The evaluation design was descriptive and process-oriented, relying on systematic recording of the project experience. For evaluation purposes, assessments of the children involved four diagnostic instruments, interviews, school reports and behavioral indicators. The only statistical finding reported was that 70% of the 50 children discharged from the program were placed in permanent situations in the community. Thirty percent were still in need of specialized institutional care. As with the Rubenstein et a1. articles, the authors indicated that the $23 per day cost "compared favorably" with institutional care costs during that year, making the program cost- effective. 46 Hawkins et a1. (1985) discussed the PRYDE model of therapeutic foster care. This program placed children ranging in age from 4 to 18 years in therapeutic foster care due to aggressive, attention-getting, hyperactive, or unethical behavior. The program's results were measured from three perspectives: (a) discharge rates: 82% of the 16 children were discharged to less restrictive, less structured settings: (b) recidivism: of 28 children discharged, only 1 had reentered the child welfare or juvenile court system 6 months after discharge; and (c) self-report: satisfaction questionnaires completed by the youths in the program revealed that 85% of their statements were "very positive about their foster parents." Hazel (1982) reviewed the Kent Family Placement Project. Adolescents ranged in age from 14 to 18 years and consisted of three groups: delinquent boys, girls with unacceptable or dangerous behavior (though not necessarily illegal), and casualties of the system (i.e., children who were being stored in institutions). The author indicated that an independent study (not further defined) of 25 placements, as well as the Project's own evaluation (not further clarified) of 156 boys and girls placed in the program, revealed that three-fourths of them "improved during placement. " This "improvement" was not 47 specifically defined, nor was any information provided regarding the design or measurement instruments used. Lanier and Coffey (1981) discussed the specifics of the therapeutic foster care program developed at the Smokey Mountain Area Mental Health Center» Ln this article they merely described the services offered and did not indicate the number or criteria of children served. Furthermore, no statistical or evaluative outcome data were noted. The authors did comment that their approach was cost-effective, although no supportive documentation was provided. In summary, only one comparative study on the efficacy of therapeutic foster care appeared in the literature (Rubenstein et al., 1978) . Furthermore, in that study no significant difference in improvement was found between children in therapeutic foster care and those placed in residential facilities. However, the authors did contend that therapeutic foster care was more cost-effective than residential treatment, a finding reiterated by Larson et a1. (1978) and Lanier and Coffey (1981). may Psychoeducation, which has emerged from the coupling of the fields of psychology and education, does not follow the traditional medical model but instead has a format of 48 intervention beginning with client dissatisfaction, moving to the setting of goals and the instruction of specific skills, and culminating in particular goal achievement. A specific skills training program within the field of psychoeducation is problem solving, which has been used successfully with various psychiatric populations, including emotionally disturbed and delinquent adolescents. Problem-solving theory contends that ineffective coping with problem-solving situations, along with accompanying personal and social consequences, may be the necessary and sufficient condition for the development of an emotional or behavioral disorder requiring psychological treatment. An additional contention is that specific skills training in problem solving may be necessary. One such specific training program is the Personal Paradigm Shift, used in this study. A treatment setting in which an examination of the problem-solving skills of the population is lacking is therapeutic foster care, where adolescents are placed with specially trained foster parents who function as change agents. Although the literature is substantial regarding descriptions of therapeutic foster care programs, few data exist on identifying the specific population being served by this treatment modality. Filling a portion of this data gap by identifying some of the characteristics of this population was the focus of this study. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY This chapter contains a discussion of the therapeutic foster care population, the sample-selection criteria, and the representative sample that was examined in this study. Additionally, procedures and instrumentation used in this study are explained. The section on the Millon Adolescent Personality Inventory (MAPI) includes a description of the instrument, its construction, scoring, reliability and validity, as well as a review of the research on this instrument. The discussion of the Personal Change Questionnaire contains a description of the instrument, including information regarding its reliability and validity. Additionally, a brief overview is given of the survey developed for this study. A presentation of the data analysis and design of the study, followed by a summary, concludes Chapter III. Sa e Popularion Child and Family Services is a private, nonprofit organization with a central office in Okemos, Michigan. 49 50 Fourteen statewide branches accept foster care referrals from various private and public agencies throughout Michigan. Ten of those branches have therapeutic foster care programs, with a total population of approximately 250 children placed in therapeutic foster care at any give time. Nine of those ten branches agreed to participate in this study, representing' approximately’ 220 children in therapeutic foster homes. Sample-Selegrion Crireria The criteria for selecting the adolescents for this study were as follows: 1. The adolescent must be between the ages of 13 and 18 years. 2. The adolescent must be placed within the therapeutic foster care program at the agency (as opposed to being in regular foster care). 3. The adolescent must have at least an average IQ and/or be able to read at a sixth-grade level or better in order to complete the instruments used in this study. From the pool of adolescents meeting those criteria, a sample of 100 adolescents was randomly selected. A return rate of 94% from the adolescents' therapists was obtained for the demographic information on these adolescents. However, a final voluntary subject 51 population on the MAPI and Personal Change Questionnaire consisted of 41 adolescents. Description of the Sample Descriptively speaking, of the 94 adolescents initially surveyed in therapeutic foster care for this study, the following results were obtained. With regard to race, 86 were CaucasiansAnglo, 5 were Hispanic, 1 was Native American, and 2 were biracial. Forty-nine were male and 44 were female: one adolescent’s gender was not noted on the survey. With regard to state or court ward- ship, 62 adolescents were temporary wards, 14 were perma- nent wards (meaning parental rights had been terminated), and 18 were missing data on the survey for this variable. Concerning family status, 51 were biological offspring, 5 were adopted, and 38 were missing data on the survey for this variable. The mean age was 15.65 years. The mean length of time in therapeutic foster care was 17.7 months, and the mean length of time in regular foster care pro- grams was 4.2 months. The mean number of previous foster care placements was 1.6. The mean score by the therapists of the adolescents’ overall functioning on a scale of 0 (poor) to 7 (very well) was 4.5. A statistical comparison was run between the 41 (n = 41, Group B) adolescents who agreed to participate and the 59 (r = 59, Group A) who did not complete their 52 instruments. A total of eight different demographic and descriptive variables (i.e., age, gender, race, whether or not the adolescent was a ward of the court or state, length of time the adolescent had been in therapeutic foster care, number of previous foster care placements, whether the adolescent had ever been institutionalized, and the therapist’s rating--on an 8-point scale--of how well the adolescent dealt with problems) were examined. The analysis revealed that, except for gender, there were no significant differences between the two groups. A further statistical analysis indicated that gender had no significant correlation with any of the variables of interest in this study (i.e., the adolescent’s self-rating of problem-solving ability, the therapist’s rating of the adolescent’s problem-solving ability, the length of time in therapeutic foster care, or any of the four components of‘ the Personal Change. Questionnaire). This suggests that, for the purposes of this study, the final sample was representative of the original sample, with no noticeable biasing. Complete descriptive data on the study sample are presented in the Results section. Procedures For each adolescent, the branch agency received one descriptive survey for the therapist to complete, as well 53 as one MAPI and one Personal Change Questionnaire for the adolescent to complete. The adolescents were given the following written instructions: You were selected to be a part of a study of teenagers in special foster care programs. Your participation is completely voluntary. You can stop being in this study any time you want. The study consists of two questionnaires that will take you approximately one hour to complete. You won’t be putting your name on these forms and no one will know which answers were yours. Your therapist will be available to answer any questions you might have. Are you willing to be a part of this study by completing these forms? Upon receiving a verbal consent, the adolescent was then provided with the two instruments (the MAPI and the Personal Change Questionnaire), both of which contain instructions for completion. With consultation from the branch agencies, an additional statement was developed to give to the adolescent as an explanation for the study: We are asking you to complete these forms to help us find out how adolescents in foster care programs like yours feel and think. Once we know that, we may be able to be more helpful. You will notice that one of the forms asks you to pick a behavior of yours that has created problems for you, like talking back to teachers, not talking about your feelings, not getting your chores done, or not completing your homework. Once you have picked the problem, just answer the questions on the form. Maybe the behavior you pick is one that you are working on as a goal in your foster home, with your caseworker or counselor, or at school. There is no right or wrong answer to these questions. Just answer as honestly as you can. If you need help in deciding on a problem or goal, you can ask your foster parents, caseworker, or counselor for help. Thank you for helping us by answering these questions. 54 The reference to the Personal Change Questionnaire specifically in this statement was due to the resistance expressed by the therapists to administer this instrument to the adolescents because of its stance of self- responsibility regarding behavior. Some therapists strongly verbalized that the adolescents were victims and not responsible for the problems for which they were receiving treatment. The statement allowed the adolescents to select a behavior that they perceived as creating problems for them. This will be discussed further with reference to the results in the Discussion section. Wish e ' o o esce t P s ' vento The MAPI consists of 150 true-false items designed to measure objectively certain personality dimensions, expressed concerns, and behavioral correlates in adolescents between the ages of 13 and 18 years. (See Appendix A.) The scores on the profiles are adjusted for prevalence data on each of the 20 scales, using normative scores. The eight personality variables are derived from Millon’s (1969, 1981) theory of personality, which consists of a 2 x 4 matrix (passive/active x detached/ dependent/independent/ambivalent). 55 The first dimension pertains to the pattern of coping behavior employed by the adolescent to maximize rewards and minimize pain. An active pattern means that the adolescent appears aroused and attentive, arranging and manipulating life events to obtain gratification and avoid discomfort. A passive pattern results in an apathetic, restrained, yielding, or resigned demeanor that suggests the adolescent is content to allow events to take their own course without personal attempts at regulation or control. The second dimension revolves around the primary source from which the adolescent attempts to gain comfort and satisfaction (positive reinforcement) or to avoid emotional pain and distress (negative reinforcement). The detached type experiences few rewards or satisfactions in life, either from self or others. The dependent type measures personal satisfactions or discomforts by how others react. The independent type relies on personal values and desires, with little regard to the concerns and wishes of others. Finally, the ambivalent type feels conflicted over whether to follow others or be guided by personal desires and needs. The eight resulting personality styles are as follows: 56 Scale 1: Introversive (31 items): Passive-Detached: is quiet, unemotional, indifferent about involvement with others. Scale 2: Inhibited (41 items): Active-Detached: is withdrawn, mistrustful, lonely, fearful of rejection. Scale 3: Cooperative (35 items): Passive-Dependent: seeks relationships in which others provide support, lacks initiative or autonomy, is clinging. Scale 4: Sociable (29 items): Active-Dependent: intensely needs attention and approval, is dramatically emotional, superficial, capricious, manipulative. Scale 5: Confident (42 items): Passive-Independent: is self-assured, has high self-esteem, is self- centered, exploitive, takes others for granted. Scale 6: Forceful (37 items): Active-Independent: intensely' needs jpower' and. control, is suspicious, hostile, angry. Scale 7: Respectful (29 items): Passive-Ambivalent: is serious-minded, rule-conscious, subservient with underlying anger and opposition, fearful of disapproval. Scale 8: Sensitive (46 items): Active-Ambivalent: is pessimistic, emotionally labile, explosively angry alternating with apologetic. The following eight scales on the MAPI are considered "expressed. concerns," which focus upon the affect and cognitions that adolescents may experience about issues that tend to concern this age group at one time or another during this developmental stage. The intensity with which the adolescents experience these issues is reflected in the score elevations of each scale. Scale A: Self-Concept (36 items): clarity of identity--who one is and what one will become. 57 Scale B: Personal Esteem (36 items): level of satisfaction experienced by the adolescent when comparing self against an ideal. Scale C: Body Comfort (21 items): comfort with body maturation and adolescent developmental changes. Scale D: Sexual Acceptance (28 items): satisfaction with sexuality and development of heterosexual relationships. Scale E: Peer Security (23 items): degree of acceptance and sense of belonging with peers. Scale F: Social Tolerance (26 items): interpersonal sensitivity and respect for others. Scale G: Family Rapport (25 items): degree of comfort within the family system. Scale H: Academic Confidence (30 items): attitudes toward academic achievement. The MAPI also contains four behavioral correlates. These have been assessed through empirically derived scales rather than self-report. High scores on these scales suggest the respondents answered similarly to adolescents with poor impulse control, difficulty conforming to rules and norms, poor academic achievement, or school truancy. The scales are labeled as follows: Scale SS: Impulse Control (35 items). Scale TT: Societal Conformity (39 items). Scale UU: Scholastic Achievement (41 items). Scale WW: Attendance Consistency (36 items) Sggrrgg_rn§_u521. When constructing the MAPI, Millon (1982) believed that his personality inventory measured traits, concerns, and behaviors that were neither normally 58 distributed nor of equal prevalence in normal and clinical populations. Accordingly, Millon maintained that transforming the raw scores into standard scores was inappropriate. Accordingly, when scoring the MAPI, raw scores are transformed into base rate scores. This conversion was determined by personality trait prevalence data obtained in external validation studies on 430 individuals. Two arbitrary numbers were selected to designate the two base rate cutting lines. A base rate score of 75 indicated the "presence" of the personality trait, concern, or behavior. Adolescents scoring above 74 are said to possess, to a clinically significant degree, the trait, concern, or behavior assessed. Similarly, a base rate score of 85 indicated the "predominance" of the personality, trait, or behavior. Adolescents scoring above 84 on a scale are considered to display that trait, concern, or behavior as a dominant element of their clinical picture. A base rate score of 50 was selected to represent the median for all adolescents who participated in the test-construction studies. Within the MAPI instrument are items to assess reliability and validity of responses. Three factors contribute to nonusable responses: 59 1. Tendencies of the adolescent to "deny" emotional problems, trying to appear healthier than would be objectively determined. 2. Tendencies of the adolescent to "complain" excessively, trying to appear more disturbed than is objectively justified. 3. Tendencies of the adolescent to respond "randomly," i.e., not answering, either by accident or intentionally, in a manner reflecting comprehension and relevance. For the reliability index, items were included to focus directly on the adolescent’s belief that his/her responses represent an enduring set of feelings and that he/she took the testing situation seriously. Three items that comprise the reliability index (Scale 21) have been shown to be very successful in identifying those adolescents for whom test-taking attitudes or lack of conviction reflected in their responses resulted in scores with little enduring meaning. A score of 2 is recorded as unreliable, 0 equals reliability, and 1 is questionably reliable. The validity index (Scale 22) targets problems arising from noncomprehension or random responding. These problems seem to occur, according to the manual, when the adolescents are too disoriented to focus or maintain interest in the inventory or are purposely avoiding 60 committing themselves due to their perception of consequences associated with the test” .Accordingly, implausible but nonbizarre items were included that had a frequency response of less than .01 among normal or clinical populations. Seventy-five percent of all adolescents who fail to complete the MAPI carefully or relevantly are detected by the three items in this index, which is highly sensitive to careless, confused, or random responding. Again, scores of 2 indicate unreliability, 0 suggests reliability, and 1 indicates questionable reliability. uat o e ’abi i a d vali 't . To develop a diagnostic instrument capable of efficiently assessing adolescents on a number of significant personality and behavioral characteristics, the MAPI was constructed with attention directed to reliability as well as internal structure and external validation. Empirical evaluation of the MAPI consists of (a) reliability estimates for test-retest stability and internal consistency, (b) scale intercorrelations, (c) external correlations with other personality instruments, and (d) results of factor analyses. The reliability and validity of the MAPI instrument have been carefully investigated by other researchers, using a variety of established empirical procedures, 61 including test-retest and internal consistency indicators of reliability and convergent validity with other diagnostic inventories. The procedures used and results obtained are described in detail in Millon, Green & Meagher (1982). Researgn on rne MAPI. Because the MAPI is a relatively new instrument, little research was available for review. Only one study to date has appeared in the literature regarding the use of the MAPI as a validating instrument (Levine, Green, & Millon, 1986). To establish external-criterion validation, 181 adolescent subjects completed the MAPI and the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA), which is based on Mahler’s theory of early childhood separationsindividuation processes. An examination of the results by Levine et a1. supported the establishment of external validity for the MAPI. Other research has been conducted on the MAPI and presented at various conferences, printed in unpublished reports at psychiatric institutions, and written as dissertation findings. In her dissertation, Fons (1987) used the MAPI to compare 30 adolescents in a psychiatric inpatient unit to two groups of 30 high school students (one group of high school students having scored in the abnormal range on the MAPI pretest and one group having scored in the normal range). Fons found a greater degree 62 of test-retest stability in the personality scales of the MAPI than in the Expressed Concern scales. She was also able to demonstrate that scores on the Inhibited and Sensitive scales decreased with treatment, that the Sociable and Confident scales scores increased with treatment, that the Expressed Concern scales decreased with treatment, and that the untreated groups evidenced no significant differences on pre-posttest scores. Five presentations examined the MAPI at the Millon conference in Miami, Florida, in March 1986 (Fons, 1987). The following is a summary of those articles: Zupkus (1986) discussed her research in the use of the MAPI to differentiate among normal, emotionally disturbed, and delinquent adolescents. With 60 subjects in each group, membership was predicted with 60.5% accuracy among all three groups and with 76.1% accuracy between the normal and pathological groups. Additionally, Zupkus found that Forceful and Sensitive personality styles were more frequent among maladjusted adolescents than among normals. Watchman (1986) presented research data that used the MAPI to differentiate between depressed and nondepressed adolescents. His results indicated that high scorers on the Introversive, Cooperative, and Respectful scales used denial as a coping mechanism and changed very little when 63 depressed; high. scorers. on 'the Forceful and Sensitive scales became more aggressive when depressed: and high scorers on the Inhibited, Sensitive, and Cooperative scales became highly intropunitive. The findings suggested that personality style as measured by the MAPI is a salient factor in determining an adolescent’s response to coping with depression. Tracy (1986) reported results from research using the MAPI with 230 adolescent inpatients and outpatients. He warned that the MAPI’s weakness tends to rest in the area of diagnosis of the more severe Axis I pathology, including psychosis and major depression. Pantle and Wassink (1986) also employed the MAPI with inpatient adolescents selected from the crisis treatment unit of a psychiatric hospital. They found no significant difference using the MAPI between the groups who had threatened suicide and those who had demonstrated no suicidal behavior before admission. However, the MAPI did differentiate between those who had attempted suicide and those who had not, with the group attempting suicide having significantly higher scores on Inhibited and Sensitive and significantly lower scores on Sociable and Confident. Using the MAPI, Pantle and Houskamp (1986) assessed differences among adolescents with a history of sexual abuse. Three groups of adolescents, matched by age, 64 gender, and diagnosis, were equally divided among those having experienced documented severe sexual abuse, moderate sexual abuse, and no sexual abuse. Their results indicated that MAPI profiles on adolescents who had experienced sexual abuse differed from the profiles of adolescents who had not been sexually abused. The Inhibited, Personal Esteem, Peer Security, and Social Tolerance scales were significantly higher in the two sexually abused groups, whereas the Sociable and Confident scales were significantly lower. Furthermore, as the severity of the sexual abuse increased, so did the scores on the Peer Security and Social Tolerance scales, whereas scores on the Sociable and Confident scales declined. Pantle and Houskamp interpreted those findings to mean that severely abused adolescents perceived themselves as less confident and outgoing and as having greater difficulty with peer relationships and tolerance for others. The MAPI also has been used in two unpublished reports researched at Pinerest Christian Hospital in Grand Rapids, Michigan, in 1988. Using the MAPI, VanZytveld (1988) assessed 56 adolescents characterized by differential levels of suicide proneness. The three groups being compared were those who had threatened suicide, those who had attempted suicide, and those who 65 had no history of suicidal behavior. Significant differences were found among the groups. Specifically, those who had threatened suicide scored highest on the Inhibited and Sensitive scales and lowest on the Sociable and Confident scales. Additionally, on the Expressed Concern scales, the threateners scored highest on having issues regarding Self-Concept, Personal Esteem, Body Comfort, Sexual Acceptance, Peer Security, and Social Tolerance. Those who had attempted suicide showed significantly less distress on the MAPI relative to those who had threatened suicide. VanZytveld suggested that the difference might have been due to the attempters seeing themselves as having made a choice and, thus, having achieved some type of resolution. On the other hand, the threateners could be seen as being in the midst of indecision and, thus, of distress that is reflected in the MAPI scales. The other research study conducted at Pinerest. was performed by Houskamp (1988) and used the MAPI to assess differences between a sexual-abuse group and a control group with no history of sexual abuse. Significant differences were found on the Inhibited, Sociable, Confident, Personal Esteem, Peer Security, and Social Tolerance scales. As noted in other research cited herein, scores on the scales correlate with the severity 66 of the abuse, indicating a possible relationship between severity of sexual abuse and its effects. In a paper presented at the Society of Personality Assessment in New Orleans, Louisiana, in March 1988, Trenerry, Pantle, and Ziebelman (1989) compared the MAPI with the Rorschach in assessing 63 adolescents admitted to an inpatient crisis unit. They found a significant correlation between the Depression index and the Inhibited, Cooperative, Sociable, Confident, and Forceful Personality scales. Furthermore, they found a constellation of MAPI Personality scales that demonstrated predictive value regarding suicidal behavior. Pantle (1989) described a comparison of the MAPI with the Rorschach and the Multiscore Depression Inventory in a paper presented at the Society for Personality Assessment in New York in April 1989. Pantle performed the comparison to assess mood disorders of adolescents hospitalized for depression at the Pinerest Christian Hospital in Grand Rapids, Michigan. Pantle reported that an elevation in the Forceful scale may be indicative of "masked depression" that presents as disruptive behavior. In line with Millon’s theories, Pantle’s research further suggests that passive-aggressive and avoidant types are susceptible to depressive disorders, whereas histrionic, narcissistic, and antisocial personality styles tend to be 67 intolerant of negative affect and often take a "flight into health" shortly after being admitted for treatment. In summary, published research on the MAPI is sparse. However, research findings presented at conferences and summarized in dissertations and unpublished reports have supported its use in assessing various adolescent populations. In particular, the MAPI has been found to have assessment value in terms of depression, suicidal proneness, and sexual abuse. These findings would suggest the appropriateness of using the MAPI with the adolescent population in this study. The Personal Change Questionnaire The Personal Change Questionnaire consists of 38 questions. (See Appendix B.) Respondents are asked to answer on a scale from 0 (very poor) to 7 (very well). The first 37 questions relate to the process of problem solving and are based on the four stages of the Personal Paradigm Shift: (a) awareness; (b) decision making, (c) oppositional alternatives, and (d) treatment planning and action. The final question (#38) asks the respondents to rate themselves on their own problem-solving ability. (A corresponding question regarding the respondent’s problem- solving ability is asked of the therapist on the Survey instrument used in this study.) 68 W- The Personal Change Questionnaire’s four stages correspond to identical stages in the Personal Paradigm Shift. Each stage in the Personal Paradigm Shift consists of detailed steps designed to educate the respondent about the relationships between external behaviors and internal experiences, such as thoughts, feelings, and bodily reactions. Because the Personal Change Questionnaire is based directly on the Personal Paradigm Shift, the items on the questionnaire correlate with the individual steps of the Personal Paradigm Shift. The following is a description of the stages as they relate to the items on the Personal Change Questionnaire: Stage One: Awareness (Items 1-10). The first stage is designed to guide respondents through a self-analysis of their present behaviors to discover the personal dynamics of the present problem. The focus is on identifying the problem and its antecedent or concurrent thoughts and feelings. The intention is that respondents become aware of and examine relationships between internal and external variables that create and maintain their personal problems. stage Two: Decision-Making (Items 11-17). The second stage is based on awareness gained in Stage One. The focus is on determining how the information from Stage One influences their decisions about making changes. The 69 problem-solving process at this stage consists of three steps: constructing a balance sheet of thoughts and feelings surrounding personal changes, making a commitment to change, and confronting the barrier of fear about changing. Stage Three: Oppositional Alternatives (Items 18- 26) . The third stage aims at helping the respondents replace old internal reactions with new thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. In addition, this stage attempts to foster a sense of self-control over internal reactions and subsequent behaviors. Stage Four: Treatment Planning and Action (Items 27- 37). This final stage provides realistic steps for developing and carrying out plans to bring about desired changes and solving the problem identified in Stage One. The problem-solving process in this stage consists of goal setting, finding appropriate role models and support groups, and creating specific methods of record-keeping and self-evaluation. t va ' 't . The Personal Change Questionnaire is new and, at the time of this study, was being researched in terms of its reliability, content validity, and construct validity (Kelley, in process). Kelley (1989) noted that the reliability of the Personal Change Questionnaire could be evaluated in terms of 70 Cronbach’s alpha. The content validity relied on the use of expert judges regarding the items on the questionnaire. To assess construct validity, Kelley stated that the clients were to be given a self-rating and a therapist’s rating, which were subjected to a correlational analysis. Kelley’s (1991) preliminary results suggested that the Personal Change Questionnaire did demonstrate good reliability in terms of its internal consistency. Kelley found the Personal Awareness scale to have a Cronbach’s alpha of .83, the Decision-Making scale to have a .81 score, the Oppositional Alternatives scale to have a .92 score, and the Planning and Action scale to have a Cronbach’s alpha of .92, thus indicating good internal consistency for each of the four scales in the instrument. In addition, in terms of construct validity, Kelley found that the clients’ self-ratings and the therapists’ ratings of problem-solving ability were in fact positively correlated (r = .44, p = .001) , thus demonstrating the validity of that aspect of the Personal Change Questionnaire. Because the original Personal Change Questionnaire targeted adult clients, minor alterations were made for this study in vocabulary and with regard to examples used, in order to enhance readability and comprehension of the items by an adolescent population. Two methodological 71 safeguards were incorporated to ensure that the resulting instrument could be appropriately completed by this target population. First, the instrument was pilot-tested with a sample of 10 adolescents (5 male and 5 female) randomly selected by the staff of St. Vincent Home for Children in Lansing, Michigan. This group was selected because of their similarity to the target population of this study. Adolescents from the St. Vincent Home frequently move into therapeutic foster care programs. The adolescents in the pilot test were all able to successfully read and complete the test instrument. As a second check, a panel of professionals who work with this population was selected to review the instrument and make suggestions regarding wording and comprehension. The only suggestion received from the panel was that the word "counseling" be substituted for "therapy." This change was made in the instrument provided to the adolescents in this dissertation study. Although comprehensive reliability and validity testing of the Personal Change Questionnaire was well beyond the scope of this study, certain methodological procedures were employed to address these issues. In particular, the four Personal Change Questionnaire stages were examined using a combination of rational and empirical criteria, including (a) logical fit of each item with its scale, (b) significant correlation of each item 72 with its scale, (c) overall scale internal consistency (using Cronbach’s alpha), (d) no substantive improvement in scale internal consistency if the item were removed, and (e) items correlated highest with their own scale. The scales for the four stages satisfactorily met all of the first four criteria. However, a total of 6 of the 37 items did correlate slightly (although not significantly) higher with another scale than with their own. The content of those six items was reexamined in terms of their logical fit with their original and competing scales. For the purposes of this study, it was determined that the original scale placement was satisfactory. The reliability of each of the four scales, in terms of internal consistency, was found to be similar to the previously cited results obtained by Kelley. The Personal Awareness scale demonstrated a Cronbach’s alpha of .80, the Decision-Making scale was .78, the Oppositional Alternatives scale was .84, and the Planning and Action scale was .91, thus indicating that each of the four scales demonstrated good internal consistency. Finally, there was one opportunity for directly examining the issue of validity in this study. Item 38 of the Personal Change Questionnaire assesses the adolescents’ perceptions of their overall problem-solving 73 ability. Similarly, the survey completed by the adolescents’ therapists for this study included an item measuring the therapists’ assessment of the adolescents’ problem-solving ability. The results showed that the correlation of the two different measures was statistically significant (r = .40, p ==.01), indicating at least that degree of external criterion validity for the Personal Change Questionnaire. Once again, this result is very similar to the previously cited results obtained by Kelley. On the basis of the prior research with the Personal Change Questionnaire, together with the pilot-testing and expert review conducted of preliminary procedures, it appears that the instrument used was reliable, valid, and appropriate for the target population examined in this study. $11M! The final instrument used in this study was the survey to ascertain certain demographics of the population in therapeutic foster care. (See Appendix C.) The instrument was given to the adolescent’s therapist to complete and return. The questions on the survey included age, gender, race, family status (biological or adopted, temporary or permanent ward), length of time in therapeutic foster care 74 programs, length of time in regular foster care programs, number of previous foster care placements, and history of residential placement or psychiatric hospitalizations. The final question asked the therapist to rate the adolescent on his/her problem-solving ability at the current time, using a scale of 0 (very poor) to 7 (very well). This instrument was mailed to the therapist assigned to each of the adolescents selected for inclusion in this study. It was successfully completed and returned for all of the subjects included in the final study results. 51 n The design of this study consisted of an observational analysis of the population in therapeutic foster care using the demographic information on the survey, the personality profiles suggested by the MAPI, and the examination of problem-solving abilities indicated by the Personal Change Questionnaire. Because this study involved observational analyses rather than an experimental or quasi-experimental design to measure the effects of an experimental treatment, most typical concerns regarding threats to validity were not pertinent. Nevertheless, one factor of concern in the design was the relative newness of the MAPI and the Personal Change Questionnaire. Both instruments have been developed only 75 recently and, therefore, have not been extensively researched regarding reliability and validity. Caution, accordingly, was exercised in terms of interpreting the results. Wis The analysis of the therapists’ survey featured basic descriptive statistics, including the frequencies, means, and standard deviations of the various variables. Cross- tabulations were made to develop frequency tables and descriptive graphs. The primary research instruments, i.e., the MAPI and the Personal Change: Questionnaire, ‘were first. analyzed using similar descriptive statistics. (Results for the MAPI were structured according to the protocols suggested by the developers of that instrument.) Gender differences on the Personal Change Questionnaire were examined using two-tailed t-tests and on the MAPI, using the Pearson chi- square statistic. Differences between the study sample of adolescents and the national normative data were examined using the Pearson chi-square test for goodness of fit. In each case, a significance level of .10 was selected due to the small sample sizes available and the exploratory nature of this study. In addition, a variety of correlational analyses were conducted among and between variables in the two 76 instruments, and between these instruments and the descriptive and demographic variables obtained from the therapists’ survey. Finally, Pearson correlations were used to examine the relationship between time in therapeutic foster care and results on the MAPI and the Personal Change Questionnaire. All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) computer software. Summary A sample of adolescents in therapeutic foster care was observed relative to a demographic survey, the Millon Adolescent Personality Inventory, and the Personal Change Questionnaire. The MAPI was designed to assess the overall make-up of an adolescent’s personality, including coping styles, expressed concerns, and behavioral patterns. The Personal Change Questionnaire analyzes problem-solving abilities in terms of personal awareness, decision-making skills, knowledge of oppositional alternatives, and planning and implementation behaviors. The research questions were examined through means, frequencies, cross-tabulations, and Pearson correlations. CHAPTER IV DATA ANALYSIS In Chapter IV, the results of the study are presented. Initially, the results of the descriptive survey are provided in order to identify the therapeutic foster care adolescent population studied. Thereafter, the results on each of the five research questions are presented. Specifically, the following contents are included: W: the results of the MAPI in an attempt to ascertain if there is a personality profile of adolescents in therapeutic foster care. Reseereh Questien z: the results of the Personal Change Questionnaire in an attempt to determine if adolescents in therapeutic foster care are aware of elements necessary for personal change or problem solving. Beseerch Queer; on 3 : the correlation between the results on the MAPI and the length of time in therapeutic foster care. 5 h e ' : the results on correlational data regarding the relationship between the length of time in therapeutic foster care and the results on the Personal Change Questionnaire. s u 'o : the correlation between the results on the MAPI and the results on the Personal Change Questionnaire. This presentation of information is followed at the end of the chapter by a summary of the results on the 77 78 descriptive survey, as well as all five research ques- tions. Resulrs sc ' tiv u e es 1 s The results of the descriptive survey cover the 41 adolescent subjects in therapeutic foster care who completed all of the instruments involved in this study. Sixteen of these adolescents were male; 24 were female. The data on one survey were incomplete with regard to gender. Ninety-five percent of the sample was Caucasian-Anglo, and 5% was Hispanic. Twenty-nine percent of the adolescents were temporary wards of the court or state, six were permanent wards, and data were incomplete on the remaining six. Thirty-seven percent of the adolescents had a history of psychiatric hospitalization or residential treatment. The mean age was 15.8 years. The mean length of time in the therapeutic foster care program was 16.9 months, and the mean length of time in regular foster care placements prior to admission into therapeutic foster care was 8 months. The number of previous foster care placements before entering therapeutic foster care was 1.6. The data on 5 of these 41 adolescents were omitted in the analyses when the results on the MAPI indicated that their profiles were invalid. This removal of five subjects was not considered to have posed a threat to the 79 data analysis (as discussed earlier, in the Methods section). es e ' on t of' d sc 5 Therepeutie Foster Qere The possibility of a personality profile of adolescents in therapeutic foster care was assessed via the MAPI. In examining the results of the MAPI, all 20 scales were used. When constructing the MAPI, Millon prepared national normative data, for both males and females, for each of the first eight scales. The summary scores from these normative data were obtained for use in this study via a personal telephone conversation with Millon’s offices (Kevin Anderson, National Computer Systems Professional Assessment Services, October 1, 1990). In terms of these first eight scales, the results of this study were compared to the national normative sample, as well as undergoing an analysis by gender. The remaining 12 scales do not have any normative data available. Accordingly, a comparison was made only between the males and females within this study. Per the scoring instructions of the developers of the MAPI instrument, the data have been divided into the percentage of adolescents who scored at 75 or above, indicating the "presence" of that trait, and those who scored at 85 or above, indicating the "predominance" of 80 that trait. In addition, a total score combining the presence and predominance scores has been provided for both the male and female populations. These data are presented in Graphs 1 through 20. For the first scale, Introversive, the normative female population scored at 9.6% for presence, whereas the therapeutic foster care females scored at 4.5% (see Graph 1). For predominance, the normative sample scored 7.4%, whereas the females in therapeutic foster care scored 9.1%. Thus, the total combined score reflects a similar percentage for the normative sample (17%) as for the subjects in this study (13.6). For the male population, the results on this scale were even closer. In terms of the presence of that trait, the normative and therapeutic foster care males scored identically (7.7%). For predominance, the scores were almost identical (norm group: 8.5%; study group: 7.7%). This resulted in a total combined score of 16.2% for the normative group and 15.4% for the therapeutic foster care males. Together, these results suggest little difference between the study population and the national normative group on this variable, for either males or females. When a comparison is made between males and females on being Introversive, the females have a similar total score to the males in the normative and study samples 81 .oacon e>amuo>onusH ecu so muHsmeu uo somwuoeaoo ”H ensue mmGMQMmmUMHN M>H MKQ>O-B2H . N>H WKW>O-F2 H W>H mmgOEH 0&9 a.H02 Uh? E02 029 E02 028 E02 029.. E02 0&9 E02 2 2 h h 2 2 .m h 2 2 an .H O O O .I m .I m mev AI m a; m.m Ta 4 s E. a.» I. L 1- O.” 1f OH mom .1 OH O.MH v.m.n III I mH .- mH t md .03.: L o.h.m ON ON 2. ON mN mN i mN On On .1 On mmmmmmmm 82 For the second scale, Inhibited, the females in the normative sample scored similarly (11.3%) to the females in therapeutic foster care (9.1%) for the presence of the trait (see Graph 2). However, the females in this study scored higher (22.7%) than the normative population (9.9%) on the predominance of this trait. Thus, the total combined score for the females in therapeutic foster care (31.8%) was greater than the total score for the normative population (21.2%). Interestingly, this was also the case for the male subjects. The normative population scored similarly on presence (9.8% vs. 7.7%) and higher on predominance (12.1% vs. 0%), resulting in a higher total score for the normative male population (21.9%) when compared to the males in this study (7.7%). Although males and females in the normative population were relatively similar on their scores on being Inhibited (21.9% and 21.2%, respectively), the females in this study scored significantly higher (31.8%) than the therapeutic foster care males (7.7%). For the third scale, Cooperative, the females in the normative population scored similarly (17.6%) to the therapeutic foster care females (18.2%) on the presence of that trait (see Graph 3). However, on the predominance of .oaoom omuwnwnsH on» so madameu mo comaumesoo "N nacho 83 ameHmHszH omaHmHszH ameHmHmzH one suoz use suoz use suoz use auoz one snoz use suoz s s m a s a a s z s a s O o J O O m m m 5.» .IL s.s oH [lg m.m 6H m.m H.m oH H.~H m.HH ma ma ma cm on an ull L a.H~ N am s.- m~ m~ mm on on on m.Hm wome emmmqwflmmwum mmmmmuum 84 .eHsOm o>wuoueeoou on» so nuasnou no sonwuceaoo "n cacao H>H8¢mmm000 0&8 BHOZ 0&9 BHOZ 2 2 h h «.mH h.N~ h.om v.Hm AduIOH OH ma ON mm On Q>Haflmmm000 0&9 BHOZ 0&8 BHOZ 2 2 h m m.¢ 5.5 IIL m.HH .Il m.ma 00:0 060 OH ma ON mm On Q>H9¢mfim000 0&9 EH02 0&8 BHOZ 2 I h m h.h v.mH I OKA N.mH 000 OH mH ON mm on 85 the characteristic, the normative population scored 13.8%, whereas the female subjects in this study scored 4.5%. Accordingly, the total combined score for the normative female population was somewhat higher at 31.4% than for the females in therapeutic foster care (22.7%). With regard to the males, the normative population scored higher than the therapeutic foster care males on both the presence and predominance of the characteristic (15.4% vs. 7.7% and 11.3% vs. 7.7%, respectively). Thus, similar to the female comparison, the total combined score was somewhat higher for the normative male population at 26.7% than for the therapeutic foster care males at 15.4%. When the genders were compared, the females scored higher than the males on this trait for both the normative population and therapeutic foster care subjects. For the fourth scale, Sociable, the female normative population scored less (10.2%) than the females in therapeutic foster care (22.7%) on presence, although on predominance the normative population scored slightly higher at 15.2% than the females in therapeutic foster care at 9.1% (see Graph 4). The combined total placed the therapeutic female population with a similar score on this trait (31.8%) to the normative population (25.4%). The same trend occurred with the males. The therapeutic foster care males scored higher than the normative males 86 .oasun dancfioom on» so muasnou uo confinoesoo mumcHoom uqmcHoom use suoz use suoz one suoz one suoz z z m m z s m s o o I m u m 5.5 . . ea c m H.m . o” - ma II. .. ma «.ma r ON i ON h.v~ .II n mm s mm «.mm . .IL . on : on m on m.~m sue wMQNQflflmuMMN "v guano HQQ‘HUOO 0&9 BH02 028 fih02 2 2 h b O A' m l .fi OH N.OH L o.mH ms t ON H.MN h.NN f mN 1 On Mammmwmm 87 on the presence of the trait (23.1% vs. 15%) but similarly on the predominance of the trait (7.7% vs. 9%, respectively). Nevertheless, as with the females, the combined total score resulted in the therapeutic foster care males scoring fairly similarly at 30.8% to the normative males at 24.7%. When the sexes were compared on the total score for this trait, there was almost no difference for either the normative or the therapeutic foster care populations. For the fifth scale, Confident, the therapeutic foster care females scored similarly to the normative female: population (9.1% 'vs. 12%, respectively) on the presence and predominance (9.1% vs. 7.4%) of the trait (see Graph 5). Thus, for the total combined score, the normative female population was very similar to the therapeutic foster care females (19.4% vs. 18.2%, respectively). With the males, the normative population scored about the same as the therapeutic foster care subjects on presence (8.9% vs. 7.7%) and slightly higher on predominance (14.2% vs. 737%). Accordingly, the normative male population had a slightly higher total combined score (23.1%) than the therapeutic foster care male subjects (15.4%). In comparing the genders, the females in therapeutic foster care scored higher than the .eHoom useoaucou on» so nuasmeu uo conauoeaou ezmaamzoo use suoz one sue: a z m a o u m 'OH I ma «.mfl ~.ma .IL ¢.aH u om .IL H.m~ . mm x on HmuO8 azmonmzou use suoz use auoz s z a m o - m a.» [In q.s H.m u ca «.4H - ma - om . mm I on OUQMQMEOUOHAH "m nacho 82mOH&200 0&8 BHOZ 0&8 EH02 2 S & & 8.8 . .Il m m H.m IIIL O.NH mmmmmmmm OH mu ON mN On 89 males in therapeutic foster care on this trait, whereas the reverse was true with the normative population. For the sixth scale, Forceful, the females in therapeutic foster care scored slightly higher than the normative females on presence (9.1% vs. 5%), predominance (13.6% vs. 8.8%), and therefore on the total combined score (22.7% vs. 13.8%, respectively) (see Graph 6). That was also true of the therapeutic foster care males. The males in this study scored higher than the normative males on the presence of this trait (30.8% vs. 10.6%). The differences were smaller for predominance (15.4% vs. 12.1%), although the trend was still present. For the total combined score, the therapeutic foster care males scored significantly higher for this trait (46.2%) than the normative population (22.7%). In terms of gender differences, the males scored higher than the females for both the therapeutic foster care and normative populations. For' the .seventh scale, Respectful, the female therapeutic foster care subjects scored slightly higher on presence (18.2% vs. 11.3%), similarly on predominance (13.6% vs. 12.4%), and slightly higher on the total combined score (31.8% vs. 23.7%) (see Graph 7). However, for the males, the therapeutic foster care subjects scored 90 0&8 BHO2 AO&&0&O& 0&8 EH02 & .oHoom Homeouom on» so nuHsnou uo sonHHsoEoo & +~.Gv ouo m.MH OH mH ON mN On nonhuman use suoz one sue: a s m a o 1 m II. N m I OH H.~H [Ir G.mH mums . ms . ow 1 mN . on mmflmmflflmmmmm no nacho qsmuomom one auoz one suoz : z m m o.m [In L 2 s.cH ll m.om OocomOHM OH mH ON mN On .eHsOn Hsuuomemem on» so ansan we sonHHmnBoo uh nacho 91 .HD&80&& mum ADM—Sum mm“ Qasmm mg 0&8 EH02 0&8 EH02 0&8 EH02 0&8 502 0&8 BHO2 0&8 EH02 2 2 & & 2 2 & & 2 2 & & o 0 Flu m m h 0 NI h . b m o S m a 3 IL Fl m H oloflmuLH VlolNld m H H H q . m H L .2 L cm on ~ ma .MN m N m N O n O n m.Hm HmmmH «mamdflflmmmum mudmwumm OH mH ON mN On 92 very similarly to the normative population on presence of the trait (7.7% vs. 6.9%) and on the predominance of the characteristic (7.7% vs. 9.3%). Accordingly, the therapeutic foster care males scored almost the same as the normative males on the total combined score (15.4% vs. 16.2%, respectively). The females in both the normative and therapeutic foster care populations scored higher than the males on this characteristic. The difference was significant in the therapeutic foster care group. For the eighth scale, Sensitive, the therapeutic foster care females scored slightly lower (9.1%) than the normative females (12.3%) on presence of the trait but higher on the predominance of the characteristic (27.3% vs. 11%, respectively) (see Graph 8). Accordingly, the therapeutic foster care females scored higher on the total combined score than the normative females (36.4% vs. 23.3%). For the males, the therapeutic foster care population scored higher than the normative population on the presence of the trait (30.8% vs. 8.5%) and the predominance of the trait (15.4% vs. 8.9%). They were significantly higher on the total combined score (46.2% vs. 17.4%) . Interestingly, the therapeutic foster care males scored higher than the therapeutic foster care females on this trait, but the reverse was true of the normative population. 93 .oHson 0>Huwnsum on» so nuasnon uo souwusesoo N>H8Hw2mm 0&8 EH02 0&8 EH02 2 2 & & v.8H m.MN +1 v.mm +|I1 one N.ov OH mH ON mN On &>H8Hm22m 0&8 EH02 0&8 EH02 2 2 & & O I m as [-3 o.HH [IL I mH v.mH 1 ON L -2 n.8N I On mudddflmmummm um nacho N>H8Hm2wm 0&8 EH02 0&8 EH02 2 2 & & 2 3 L m.NH m.om TOQOmQHM OH mH ON mN On 94 For the remaining 12 scales of the MAPI, normative data were not available. Accordingly, a comparison was made between males and females in therapeutic foster care on presence, predominance, and total combined scores. For the Self-Concept scale, the females were similar to the males on the presence of the issue (9.1% vs. 7.7%), slightly higher on the predominance (22.7% vs. 0%) and significantly higher on the total combined scores (31.8% vs. 7.7%) (see Graph 9). On the Personal Esteem scale, males scored higher than females on the presence of the issue (23.1% vs. 13.6%) but similarly on the predominance (23.1% vs. 27.3%). On the total combined score, the males scored similarly to the females (46.2% vs. 40.9%) (see Graph 10). On Body Comfort, males scored almost identically to females on both the presence of the issue (23.1% vs. 22.7%) and the predominance (7.7% vs. 9.1%). Therefore, they were nearly the same on the total combined score (31.8% vs. 30.1%) (see Graph 11). In terms of Sexual Acceptance, the males outscored the females on presence of the issue (18.2% vs. 0%), were similar on predominance of the issue (18.2% vs. 15.4%), and were higher on the total combined score (36.4% vs. 15.4%) (see Graph 12). 95 .mHoom uamosoonuHmm on» so muHsneH mo somHHsesou 8&20200I&A&m MOON—20.." @0Hfl2 OH mH ON mN On 8&202O0I&Hmm neuosem anoz 2 Ohm— OH mH ON mN On um cacao 8Afl0200l&dnm meHmamm neHo: o u m u mH I On 1 mm fl on mmdwmwufl 96 .mHson seeumm Hssonuee as» so muHsnoH uo somHHsano "6H sense :mmemm thommmm :mmemm Hszommmm sumamm Hflzommmm anuEm& mOHsS nOHmEO& ans2 neHsE0& noHss O O O I m I m I m I OH I OH I OH I mH I mH O MH I mH I ON I ON i ON .WI Hqu I mm H mm I MN I mN m.nm .1 I On I On I On + . +.II HmmmH eoqmmflflmmmmm mmmmmmum 97 .oHsum uuousou anon us» so nuHOnmu uo somHHoesoo 820&200 800m .neHssem nuHo: mmHosmm 820&200 anon neHs: _ LL mammmflamomum OH mH ON mN On "HH sense 820&200 >OOQ noHssoh neHez OH mH ON mN On .oHeou oossueeoos Hssxom us» so muHsmeH no souHueesoO "NH sense 202<8&200< Hdemm 202<8&200< Hdbxum 202¢8&&00¢ Hdbxmm 98 wwdflamh mQH52 MUHMBQM ”@Hm2 NTHQBTN mmflflz O O O O I m m m I OH OH OH O mH II. v mH .Il . N.mH I ON N OH ON ON I MN mN mN I On On On + [I «.mm m MUCH. moqfln—MEOUTHQ TUHMMTHQ 99 With reference to Peer Security, the males scored similarly to the females on the presence of the issue (23.1% vs. 18.2%). However, the females scored higher on the predominance of the issue (22.7% vs. 0%). Nevertheless, for"the ‘total combined score, the 'males outscored the females (40.9% vs. 23.1%) (see Graph 13). On Social Tolerance, the females scored higher than the males (18.2% vs. 0%) on the presence of the issue, but the males outscored the females on the predominance of the issue (15.4% vs. 4.5%). For the total combined score, the females scored slightly higher than the males (22.7% vs. 15.4%) (see Graph 14). With reference to Family Rapport, the males outscored the females on the presence of the issue (23.1% vs. 13.6%) and on the predominance (46.2% vs. 22.7%), and were significantly higher on the total combined score (69.3% vs. 36.3%) (see Graph 15). In terms of Academic Confidence, the males scored similarly to the females on the presence of the issue (7.7% vs. 9.1%) but higher on the predominance of the issue (15.4% vs. 4.5%) and, accordingly, on the total combined score (23.1% vs. 13.6%) (see Graph 16). Relative to Impulse Control, the males were similar to the females on the presence of the issue (7.7% vs. 4.5%), were higher on the predominance (15.4% vs. 4.5%) 100 .onon >8HMDbmm mmm& >uHHsOom Home as» so muHsmOH mo somHHmmEou 88H2902m mumm mwflflafih 00Hfl2 MQHMEGK 00HM2 O O O I m I m I OH I OH I mH I mH [IL 1 ON .IL I ON H.mN b NN I mN I mN [IL I On I On . 5.54 IIIIIIIIIIII. sue mososHsoomum "mH guano 88HmDUNm “mum mmHmso& moHsz OMGOWOHM 101 202422HO8 HHHaem osu so muHsmou uo 820&&<2 2AH2¢& mmHssmm meHs: OH mH ON mN On umH sense 8mom&¢2 8HH2<& sonHHsOEou Bmommsm uqsts neHesom meoz O I m I OH I mH I ON s.~e I mN I On + [l ~.Ov mmmmmdflmowmw mOHsEO& moHoz o I m IIL T oH G.ms I ms I cm L H.m~ I mm I on 0 00 .mHson cosmoHusou oHsmooos as» so muHsmmH mo somHHoesoO “OH nacho 202NOH&200 0H2mO¢0< muszH&200 0H2NO<0< 202NOH&200 0H2NO¢0< 103 nonsom ancz mchEem neHoz neHoae& noHs: O II— O O m m.v m m 4| 8.8 OH OH H O OH O mH mH .II mH mH «.mH ON ON OH H MN mN mN mN On On On HNMMH mmmmmflflmumum mudmwmum 104 and were higher on the total combined score (23.1% vs. 9%) (see Graph 17). With regard to Social Conformity, the males also outscored the females on presence (38.5% vs. 18.2%) and predominance (7.7% vs. 0%). Consequently, the males were significantly higher on the total combined score (46.2% vs. 18.2%) (see Graph 18). In terms of Scholastic Achievement, the males outscored the females on the presence of the issue (23.1% vs. 9.1%), but both genders scored 0% on the predominance of the issue. Accordingly, the males scored higher than the females on the total combined score (23.1% vs. 9.1%) (see Graph 19). Finally, with regard to Attendance Consistency, the females scored significantly higher than the males. The males scored. 0% for jpresence, predominance, and total combined score. The females scored 13.6% on presence and predominance, with a total combined score of 27.2% (see Graph 20). MW er Elements Neeeesary fer Wins On a scale of 0 to 7, adolescents in therapeutic foster care on an average rated themselves on problem- solving abilities as shown in Table 1. 105 .eHeom H028200 mmqu2H Houusou mmHsosH as» so muHsneH no WOHOEQH 00HM2 O I m O.m I OH I OH I OH W... - a I On A028200 NMHD&2H mmdflaflh MOHQ2 somHHoOEou C M.v GUCflflwEOUQHQ "pH sense 4028200 MMHDA2H noHesoh neHcS r_ mocwmflhm OH MH ON MN On .eHoom >uHsHousou HeHoom es» so muHsmou uo somHHcesoO ”OH sumac >8H220&200 .H‘HUOm 88H220&200 H‘HUOM 88H220&200 42¢ng 106 moHssom meHez neHssem noHoz meHssom neHoz O O O m O OH 5;. OH OH OH L ~.OH OH on ~.OH mm mm On On ~64 OOH 30 33% gm OH MH ON MN On 107 82W22>HH20¢ 0H8w¢4020m monamm .mHson useam>oHso< oHunsHosom as» so nuHsneu Ho somHusmsoo "OH smsuu most Hmme H.MN OH MH ON MN On 82222>NH20¢ 0H8M¢HO20m neHeamm mOHs: O O wmmmmflammmmm OH MH ON MN Om 8222M>2H20< 0H8M¢HO20M meHssem neHs: OOCOMQHM OH MH ON MN On .oHsun HosoumHnsou ousooseuus us» so uuHsnou mo souHHsesou "ON sense >02N8MHM200 202292288¢ 80228MHm200 20202N8MHM200 202440 Seederm “ ONO! 1 Heel showi I Women. «8...... °- 2®®lalmeetalweysthlnhbelerel 3®®Myparantshevemodsave good ome.lorthalrlamlly W O ®®l stay cool even wh:n I'm really angry withaomaon I ®®I have a strong need to tool like an important person. 6 Q (DI get a lot of .aatislaction in my school work 7 ® ®l enioy thinking about sex. I I sort of leaI sad when I see someone who's lonely. O G Q I'm sure oi my laelings about most things. 10 G ®l always try to do what is proper. II G (Di em a quiet end cooperative person 12 ®® I'm pretty sure I linow who I am and what I want in late. 13 ®®l lael guilty when I have to lie to a hand. M 6) 6| get so touchy that I can't tell: about certain things. 15 ®®l try hard to do well at almost everything I do. Id ® ®I become very excited or upset once a wool: or more. 17 GQWhen i get angry. I usually cool down andle laelings pass I. G) (D I'm quite sure that I am sexually attractive. I! QQI get along quite well with the otheraohlldren theme. 20 Q @l like to loilow Instructions end do what others aspect ol me 2I®®lhavemoretriendsthanloan li.aepupwith 22®®Iamvaryuneaeywhanrmauppoaed totellpaoplawhattodo. 23®®l like the way I look. 24®®ldomyvarybeatnettohurt people's laelings. 2I®®I Ioolrlorwardtogrowingupand making something oi myselt. ”Gal am moreworried about finishing things that I start then meet people. 27®®Ioendepandonmyperentstoba underetendingotme. a®®lwouldnaverussdrugsno metterwhat. 20®®Oealsanioyebla n®®nuwmumrnmnmie gentle and thoughtlul. 145 at ®®n lavarylmportantthatchildren lasrntoobaythairelders. 32®®lhaveaprsttycleorideaol whatlwahttodo. 33®®ltlseasylormetotelie soleevsntageo op.ls 34 (D Q ro like to trade bodies with someone 38 G G) I like to arrange things down to the last date 36 G) G) In this world. you either push or get shoved. 37 G) Q My social lilo is very satislying to me. 3. ®® I don't think I have as much Interest in sea as others my age. 39 @G) When someone hurts me. I try to lorget It. 40 G) (D I enjoy getting one ol the highest grades on a test. 41 Q Q My parents are very kind to me, 42 ® ® I have a strong desire to win any game I play with others. so ® (D I think I have a good build. as CD CD I have almost no close ties with others my age. so (D G) l have lalth that human nature is good. u®®ltlsaeapersonllinowlrom a distance. I usually try to avoid him. 47 @® When I don't get my way. I usually lose my tam.per “GQIhavaabatterldeeolthe ndot person I am than other taonegersdo 40®®Myhiandssaamtoturntome morethantoetharswhenthay have s. 30®®What this country reallyneads eramoreaariousanddevoted cltiaens. st®®lmeirerrioeooeeeriy. uQQIm'tuseieetmnmyeemh themirror. u®®luauallylatotharpaoplehove thalrownway. u®®rmemeusyinletseleooiel activities. u®®ldon1saamtaanowwhatlwant eutotlila. u®®0tharpeoplemyagssaammors aurathenlamelwhotheyareahd whatthaywant flQQWhonlwesayoungohlld.mv parentsleitvaryproudelme. ss®®lhevoeoteeerieoeriatheleet teliyeera. 50 G) G l otten doubt whathsrpoop are really interested in what I am saying to them. so®®somemeleewm probably heat? supportmawhenl'm an ut eI ®®I find It hard to reel sorry tor people who are always worried about things. ‘2 Q G I seem to have a problem getting along with other teenagers. 03 G) G) Thinking about sex coniuses me much oi the time. 04 ® ® I would much rather lollow someone than be the leader. as G) 0 To get ahead In this world I'm willing to push people who get in my way. so 6 0 I am pleased with the way my body has developed. 61 G) G) I can see more sides ol a problem better than others can. 00 Q 0 I would rather be almost anyplace but home. 0! G G Becoming involved In other people's problems is a waste OI time. 70 G Q I guess I‘m a compiainer who aspects the worst to happen. TI 9 @l otten do things tor no reason other than it might be tun. 72 Q Q It is not unusual to lael lonely and unwanted. 73 Q Q l teal pretty aimless and don' t know where l' m going. 14 @Q I do my best to stop anyone lrom trying to boss me. 75®®Itlsaasomaonayawmlottan start to yawn. too. Te®®lly parentsottanteilmsi‘m nogood. 11®®lamadramatlcandshowyaort olparson. 1|®®Isometimasleellamlnthie worldallalone. TI®®lreallyhatstohavamyworh pileup. eo®®lwoulomhereeoirectwnh people than avoid telling them something they don't like. at ®®rmprettylmmetureabeutaesual matters. 02®®rdretherlustlaarounddolna nothingthanwerhergoto II®®Letsolhidssaemtohaveitln terms. u®®lmengthamoatimportentthlngs aparaonoenhovaareastreng willandthadrivatogotahead. [Dosaoaasoaaoaaaaoaoooooo 194284 Ob®® At no time In my Ilia have I had any hair on my head or my body. ss®® l onehoet so stoned Iaitherirom alcohol.“ or drugsl that I don‘ t know what I'm mdoing. 07 G (D 'unishment never stoppedme omdolng r I wanted. fl®®lveryoitenthinklam not wanted by others In a group. fl®®0thars my age seam to have things together better than I do. OO®®Paopia can lniluanoa me quite easily. .1 @G) I otten iaal so angry that I want to throw and break things. s2®®lllhe ithordtoundaratand why people cry at a sad movie. 03 G G l oitan say things that I regret having said. 94 G) G I guess I depend too much on others to be helpiul to ms. 05 Q Q I’m not answering these questions honestly at all. 06 G (D I have a pretty hot temper. s7 G) G) I iaal ten but oi things socla I.ly D. G) (D I like to be the one In authority to take charge oi things. 99 ® CD I‘ve just about given up as tar as school is concerned. 100 6CD l Ilka it st home. let G) G) I don't IIIIIIII thet other teenagers are not interested in my irlandshlp. 102 G e I think teenagers are expected to know too much about sex. 103 ®® I am very pleased with all the things I have done up to new. to4 G G) Others my age never seem to call me to get together with them. I los®®lllitstotelletherseeeutthe things I have done well. 100®®lemgledthetieelinge eiIoIIt sax have become a part oi my liia new. 101®®lgatvary htanadwhen I think oi IIg ell alone in the world. los®®lryouesltsamtoeescrlee myasli I wouldn‘t know what to say. iO!®®ldon’tdependmuchonothar pacplaioririandship. tto®®l mot lrrll rhese leech oi myseli in liia. CS Trans-“tit” Maxi-1.318 1510312 146 111®® II I read these questions a month mno.w I'm sure I would change most oi my answers nz®® Tosaesomaonesuiiering doesn't bot.harme 11:66) I'm Ieelous olthe special att antion that the other children in the iamily get. 114 @G azsnpaopla are better looking Its G) C?) All my Ilia I have to 'blow up- every now and than. 110 ® G) A quiet hobby is more iun ior me than a part y 117®® I get upset when I see a very sick person 118 ® ® I get upset when things I don‘t expect happen to me. 119®® I worry about my looks. 120 G) Q I'm among the more popular kids It school 121 CD CD There are always a number oi reasons why most problems can't be solved. 122 GG I do my best to get along with others by being pleasant and agreeable 123 G) a Sex is disgusting 124 (D (D I have «own across the Atlehtic 30 times last year. 125 ®® it is good to have a regular way oi doing things so as to avoid mistakes. 126 (D (D My iemilyn. is .elways yelling and light 12'! G) G) I would like to continue In school and college as long as I can. 12. @(D I seem to lit in right away with any group oi new kids I most. 129 G) Q There's nothing I like more than tting in a car and acorn ng oil. 13006) l'vadone most things In my Iweiiavery wel.l 131®®Lonalykldsuauallydasarva 132®®lilwanttovdosomathing. liust ittwlthout thmkingoiwhat might happen. tsa®®sellrtlsolwhstlheveoorlehes bsanappracistsdbyothsrs. 134®®Ihaven't baanpaying much attantrontothaquastieneon thasapagse. 138®®lmaksnastyremarkstopsoplo lithaydaservait. 180®®Ieitenieeleslirmllestlng ereIlIIIl.sortorieetIIIllte. IatGDQ I‘m ashamed oi my body. lu®® NobodyM seems to care about his atho 130 Q G I think rrh better looking than most oi the kids I know. 14039 I'm very mature ior my I” andmknow what I want to do inl 141 ®® I like being In a crowd Inst to be with lots oi people. 142 Q G In many ways I iaal very superior to most people. 143 Q G) Most other teenagers don't seem to like me. m G) G) Most people an be trusted to be kind and thoughtiul. 14b ®® I lilts to run a lot. lee G) (D I don't really care what I'll do in Ilia. 147 G G I oiten iaal that others do not want to be iriendly to me. I48 (D (D It is very diiiicult ior ms to stop ieelings irom coming out. 149 ® ® I worry a great deal about sexual matters. 150 G) (D I can control my iaalrngs easily. FOR PROCESSING RETURN TO NATIONAL COMPUTER SYSTEMS P.O. BOX 1294 MINNEAPOLIS. MINNESOTA 55440 APPENDIX B THE PERSONAL CHANGE QUESTIONNAIRE 147 PERSONAL CHANGE QUESTIONNAIRE mm: For each statement, circle a number on how well you know yourself on a scale of 0 to 7, with 0 meaning that you "don’t know" and 7 meaning that you know "very well.” Statements refer to your' own behaviors, thoughts, feelings, goals, etc. The word "goals" refers to plans for changing your old behaviors. Don’t Know Know Hell I. I know what my main problem is. Example: I get into fights at school. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 2. I know specific goals or ways that will help me solve this problem. Example: I could handle my anger by talking, not hitting. O l 2 3 4 5 6 7 3. I know the situation in which my problem takes place. Example: I know I get angry when someone teases me. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 4. I know what I feel when this problem happens. Example: I get embarrassed when people tease me. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 5. I know what I am thinking in this problem situation. Example: I wonder if some- thing is wrong with me. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 6. I know what actions I take when these thoughts and feelings happen in the problem situation. Example: I stop hanging around with kids who tease me. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 7. I know the consequences of my actions in this problem situa- tion. Example: I know I’ll get grounded if I fight. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8. I know if I am proud or ashamed of myself in this problem situa- tion. Example: I usually put myself down for letting other kids’ words bother me. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 10. ll. l2. 13. l4. l5. 16. 17. 148 I know what I will lose if I give up my current behavior in the problem situation: Example: I lose the feeling of being strong that I get when I fight. I know what need of mine is being met by my current behav- iors in the problem situation. Example: I like the attention I get when I fight. I know what punishments I will get when I change my actions in the problem situation. Example: It would feel like a punishment to lose some of the attention I get from fighting. I know the rewards I will get when I change my actions in the problem situation: Example: If I stop fighting, I will be given extra social outings at home. I know the punishments I will get if I don’t change my actions. Example: I will lose friends if I keep fighting. I know what rewards I will get if I don’t change my actions. Example: If I keep fighting, I will be suspended and won’t have to attend school for a week. I know how much I am trying to change my actions. Example: I am willing to miss some free time to attend therapy sessions. I know the fears I will have if I don’t change my actions. Example: I may have to leave this placement and go somewhere worse. I know how to avoid my fears. Example: I can act like it doesn’t bother me that I may have to leave. ' 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 149 I know I can bring about new feelings in the problem situation. Example: I know I can feel good about myself no matter what other people say. I know I can bring about new thoughts in the problem situation. Example: I can think about making friends instead of fighting when I go outside. I know I can bring about new ways of seeing things in the problem situation. Example: I can see myself being popular and having un. I know my body could feel differ- ent in the problem situation. Example: I could feel less tense at school. I know I can have new opinions about myself in the problem situation. Example: I can think of my anger as a balloon filled with air that I can control let- ting out. I know I can learn new behaviors to solve my problems. Example: I could walk away when someone teases me. I know the consequences of my new behaviors. Example: Walk- ing away from a fight could keep me from getting grounded. I know I can see myself in a new way. Example: I can feel strong being in control of myself. I know what needs will be met by my new behavior. Example: I’ll be able to have more friends if I don’t fight with people. I know where to start to make changes. Example: I know what I need to do first to stop fighting. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 150 I know I can find someone to be helpful in making this change. Example: The staff would help me change. I know I can get information for making changes. Example: My caseworker can tell me some good ideas for controlling my temper. I know I can find a role model. Example: I know someone else who learned to control their temper. I know I can give myself rewards for making changes. Example: When I learn to control my temper, I’ll buy myself a new radio. I know I can make a plan for making these changes. Example: I can plan out my free time at school so I am too busy to be hanging around outside where I could end up fighting. I know I can give myself feedback on my progress. Example: I can keep track of my fights to see how I’m doing. I know it will take time to change. Example: If I work hard, I could probably learn to control my tem- per by my birthday. I know that I can make a backup plan if I fail to reach my goal. Example: If I lose my temper, I could decide to exercise a half hour each night so I won’t feel tense at school the next day. I know I can make a plan to avoid ruining my success. Example: Each week I could look over how I have done in controlling my temper to see if I have followed my plan. 151 37. I know I can keep a diary or chart to follow my change. Example: I can make a chart that includes how many fights I have gotten into that week. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Please give a score on how well you feel you are dealing with your problems: (Circle one.) Poorly Very Hell 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 APPENDIX C THE SURVEY 152 THE SURVEY To: Intensive Foster Care Program Branch Agency: From: Charlene Crickon Kushler Date: December ll, l989 The following adolescent has been selected for the study being conducted on Intensive Foster Care: Please complete the following information on this adolescent: Age: Race: Caucasian Black ______ Sex: M or F Hispanic Indian Family Status: (please circle) Oriental Biological or Adopted Biracial Temporary or Permanent Ward Other Length of time in Intensive Foster Care Programs: months Length of time in regular foster care programs: months Number of previous foster care placements: History of residential placement or psychiatric hospitalization: yes or no For the purpose of evaluating this adolescent for this study, please rate this adolescent on how well he/she is dealing with his/her problem at this time from your perspective, using the scale of 0 to 7 below. Poorly Very Hell 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 This survey needs to be returned by December 20, l989, to: Charlene Crickon Kushler c/o Central Office Child and Family Services 2l57 University Park Drive P.O. Box 348 Okemos, Michigan 48805 REFERENCES REFERENCES Albee, G. (1959). Mental health manpower peeds. New York: Basic Books. Authier, J., & Fix, A. (1977). A step-group therapy program based on levels of interpersonal communica- tion. Wiser. 8. 101-107. Authier, J., Gustafson, R., Guerney, B., & Kasdorf, J. (1975). The psychological practitioner as a teacher: A theoretical-historical and practical review. on - selipg Psychologist, p, 31-50. Bach, G. R., & Wyden, P. (1969). The intimage enemy. New York: William Morrow. Bauer, J. E., & Heinke, W. (1976, July-August). Treat- ment family care homes for disturbed foster children. Child Welfigge, §§, 478-490. Beck, M. A., & McDonnell, T. E. (1982). Psychoeduca- tional management of disturbed children. Egpeepipp, 102, 232- 235. Bedell, J. R., Archer, R. P., & Marlowe, H. A. (1980). A description and evaluation of a problem-solving . skills training program. In D. Upper & S. M. Ross (Eds.), eh v' ral ro t ra ° nn r 'ew. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Bierman, R., Carkhuff, R., & Santilli, M. (1969). Effi- cacy of empathic communication training groups for inner-city preschool teachers and family workers. Jou uppe; pg Applied Behaviopel Sciences, 1;, 441-446. Black, D. R., & Scherba, D. S. (1983). Contracting to problem solve versus contracting to practice behav- ioral weight loss skills. Beheyie;_1he;epy, 11, 100- 109. 153 154 Black, D. R., & Threlfall, W. E. (1986). A stepped approach to weight control: A minimal intervention and a bibliotherapy problem-solving program. Behaviop Therapy, 11, 144-157. Briscoe, R. V., Hoffman, D. B., & Bailey, J. S. (1975). Behavioral community psychology: Training a commu- nity board to problem solve. Jepzpei_ef_epp1ieg Behavioral Anaiysis, e, 157-168. Brown, R. T. (1980). Impulsivity and psychoeducational intervention in hyperactive children. Jeurpal of Leapnipg Disabilities, ig, 249-254. Brown, R. T., & Quay, L. C. (1978, January). Impulsivity and modeling in normal and behavior disordered ado- lescents. Atlanta: Georgia Educational Research Association. Carkhuff, R. (1971). Training as a preferred mode of treatment. Journal of Counseling Psychoiogy, 18, Carkhuff, R., & Banks, G. (1970). Training as a pre- ferred mode of facilitating relations between races and generations. Joprnal ef Counseling Psychology, i1, 413-418. Carkhuff, R., & Bierman, R. (1970). Training as a pre- ferred mode of treatment of parents of emotionally disturbed children. Journal of Counseling Psychol- egy, i1, 157-161. Carpenter, P., & Sugrue, D. P. (1984). Psychoeducation in an outpatient setting: Designing a heterogeneous format for a heterogeneous population of juvenile delinquents. Adeieseence, 12, 113-122. Chaney, E. P., O’Leary, M. R., & Marlatt, G. A. (1978). Skill training with alcoholics. Jeuzpai of Consul;- ing__nd_Qlin_ge1_£_xsheleg¥ 16. 1092-1104. Christoff, K. A., Scott, W. O. N., Kelley, M. L., Schlundt, D., Baer, G., & Kelly, J. A. (1985). Social skills and social problem-solving training for shy young adolescents. Beb1212r_1heraex. 16. 468- 477. Coche, E., Cooper, J. B., & Petermann, K. J. (1984). Differential outcomes of cognitive and interactional group therapies. Smeii Group Behevioz, 15, 497-509. 155 Coche, B., & Flick, A. (1975). Problem-solving training groups for hospitalized psychiatric patients. gour- ne1_9f_£§!sb91292. 21. 19-29. Collins, J. (1971). The effects of the eopjpgai {ele- 'o s ' modif'cation method on ' ommu i a ’o and adjustmenp. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University. Cox, M. J., & Cox, R. D. (1985). os ' t 'ssues ' s a ' . Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Dixon, D. N., Heppner, P. P., Petersen, C. R., & Ronning, R. R. (1979). Problem-solving workshop training. Journai of Counseiing Psychology, 26, 133-139. Donk, L. (1971). Communication. icrocounse ' ° no- vetions ip interviewing graining. Springfield, IL: C. Thomas. D’Zurilla, T. J. (1986). roblem-solv n th : sociai eompetence approach to clinical intervepeion. New York: Springer. D’Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem- solving and behavior modification. on of Appormai Peyehelogy, 18, 107-126. Edelstein, B. A., Couture, E. T., Cray, M., Dickens, P., & Lusebrink, N. (1980). Group training of problem- solving with chronic psychiatric patients. In D. Upper & S. Ross (Eds.), e vior on t ra ° An annuai peview (Vol. 2). Champaign, IL: Research Press. Ellis, A. (1961). u'de o t’ na livi . Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Ellis, A. (1963). easo d ot'o ' s ho . New York: Lyle Stuart. Ely, A. L., Guerney, B. G., & Stover, L. (1973). Efficacy of the training phase of conjugal therapy. Psychothepa py, Theogy, Reseageh end Epa egi ee, i_, 201- -207. Eysenck, H. J. (1952). The effects of psychotherapy: An evaluation. Journe1_2f_gen§ulting_2§xsn2129x. 16. 319-324. 156 Fons, B. J. (1987). d see so ve s eatmen outco e su a - cent psychiatnic inpatients. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. Foster, S. L., Prinz, R. J., & O’Leary, K. D. (1983). Impact of problem-solving communication training and generalization procedures on family conflict. thid ' eh v'or hera , 5, 1-23. Galassi, J., Galassi, M., & Litz, M. (1974). Assertive training in groups using video feedback. depnnel_ef gouneeiing Bsychplogy, 5, 390-394. Gibson, R. W. (1988). The influence of external forces on the quality assessment process. In G. Stricker & A. A. Rodriguez (Eds.), Handboeh pf gneiihy esspnence in menhal health. New York: Plenum. Giebink, J., Stover, D., & Fahl, M. (1968). Teaching adaptive responses to frustration to emotionally disturbed boys. Journa Co s tin s cholo , Q2: 366-368. Gittelman, M. (1965). Behavior rehearsal as a technique in child treatment. dounnei of thid Esyehoiogy end 21191111221. 6. 251-255. Glick, B., & Goldstein, A. P. (1987). Aggression replacement training. Jou n o ounse in nd Qevelgpmeng, pp, 356-362. Goldstein, A. P. (1973). Structuned leanning therapy. New York: Academic Press. Goldstein, A. P., Martens, B. I., Rubben, L., Van Belle, C. P., Schaaf, D., Wiersma, G., & Goedhart, R. M. (1973). The use of modeling to increase independent behavior. pehavier Beseareh end Therapy, ii, 31-42. Gordon. E- (1970). W New York: P. H. Wyden. Gormally, J., Hill, C., Otis, M., & Rainey, I. (1975). A microtraining approach to assertion training. don;- nai of Qounseiing Psyeheiogy, 22, 299-303. 157 Guerney, B., Guerney, L. F., & Stollak, G. (1971/72). The potential advantages of changing From a medical to an educational model in practicing psychology. Intenpensonai Deveiopmenn, 2, 238-245. Gum, M., Tamminen, A., & Smaby, M. (1973). Developmental guidance experiences. Personnei and Guidance dopr— al, 5;, 647-652. Gutride, M., Goldstein, A., & Hunter, G. (1973). The use of modeling and role playing to increase social interaction among asocial psychiatric patients. dournal of Censulting and Clinieai Psychoiogy, dd, 408-415. Gutride, M., Goldstein, A., & Hunter, G. (1974). Struc- ture learning therapy with transfer training for chronic inpatients. deurnal of Ciinicai Esyehoiogy, ed, 277-279. Hansen, D. J., St. Lawrence, J. S., & Christoff, K. A. (1985). Effects of interpersonal problem-solving training with chronic aftercare patients on problem- solving component skills and effectiveness of solutions. Journal of Consulting and Qiinicai Psychology, 5;. 167-174. Hawkins, R. P., Meadowcraft, P., Trout, B. A., & Luster, W. C. (1985). Foster family-based treatment. Journal of C1'nical Child Ps cholo , id, 220-228. Hazel, K. N. (1982). New hope for the teenage outcast: The family placement of disturbed and delinquent adolescents. internetional dournal of Offender hera d on ative 'mi 0 , 26. 62-71. Heppner, P. P., Hibel, J. H., Neal, G. W., Weinstein, C. L., & Rabinowitz, F. E. (1982). Personal problem-solving: A descriptive study of individual differences. Jounnel of Qonneeling Peychelogy, 22, 580-590. Hinds, W. (1983). The Peregnel Paradigm §hi£§° B lifestyle inheryention approaeh ho heeihh eene nenegenen_. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University. Houskamp, J. R. (1988). t d he ef e s o s a abuse using nhe nillon Adolescent Personality lnventony. Unpublished research report, Pinerest Christian Hospital, Grand Rapids, MI. 158 Hunt, G., & Azrin, N. (1973). A community-reinforcement approach to alcoholism. geheyion Reseerch and Thereex. 11. 91-104- Hussian, R. A., & Lawrence, P. S. (1981). Social reinforcement of activity and problem-solving training in the treatment of depressed institu- tionalized elderly patients. Co 't v h nd W. 6. 57-69- Intagliatia, J. C. (1978). Increasing the interpersonal problem-solving skills of an alcoholic population. ourna C s ' and ' 'ca s 01 489-498. Ivey, A. (1971). 'c 0 on se ' ' nnov 'o s ' t - viewing tnaining. Springfield, IL: C. Thomas. Ivey, A. (1973). Media therapy: Educational change planning for psychiatric patients. Jounnal of Ceunseiing Psychology, 29. 338-343. Jacobson, N. S. (1977). Problem-solving and contingency contracting in the treatment of marital discord. Jo n of C su ' d C 'nic s , 55, 92-100. Jacobson, N. S. (1978). Specific and nonspecific factors in the effectiveness of a behavioral approach to the treatment of marital discord. our f sult'n and Clinical Ps cholo ed, 442-452. Jannoun, L., Munby, M., Catalan, J., & Gelder, M. (1980). A home-based treatment program for agoraphobia: Replication and controlled evaluation. Beheyie; Therapy, i_, 294- -305. Jeney, J. A. (1985). ects of s hoe ucat' na ' - vent' o a' erce tions ealth behavio a psychologieel distnrhence in ehgonie pein patienhs. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. Karol, R. L., & Richards, C. S. (1978). Making treatment effects last: An investigation of maintenance strategies for smoking reduction. dennnei pf Behav- 1W. .4. 1-39- 159 Kazdin, A. E., Esveldt-Dawson, K., French, N. H., & Unis, A. S. (1987). Problem-solving skills training and relationship therapy in the treatment of antisocial child behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 5;, 76-85. Kelley, J. (1991, January & March). Personal communi- cation. Konstantareas, M., & Homatidis, S. (1984). Aggressive and prosocial behaviors before and after treatment in conduct-disordered children and in matched controls. Journel of Child Psychelogy and Psychienny, 2;, 607- 620. Krumboltz, J., & Thoresen, C. (Eds.). (1969). B a ' a counseling: Cases and technignes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Lanier, J. H., & Coffey, J. A. (1981, Winter). Therapeu- tic homes: A treatment alternative to foster care. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 48-50. Larson, G., Allison, J., & Johnson, E. (1978, January). Alberta parent counselors: A community treatment program for disturbed youths. thld Welfane, pg, 47-52. Levine, J. B., Green, c. J., & Millon, T. (1986). The separation-individuation test of adolescence. Journal of Personality Assessment, pp, 123-137. Levitt, E. (1957). Results of psychotherapy with children: An evaluation. Journal of Consulting . Psychology, gl, 189-196. Levitt, E. (1963). Psychotherapy with children: A further evaluation. Behevier Research end Thenapy, l, 45-51. Lewinsohn, P., Weinstein, M., & Shaw, D. (1969). Depres- sion: A clinical-research approach. In R. Rubin & C. Franks (Eds.), hdvaneee in hehavional therapy. New York: Academic Press. Libert, J., & Lewinsohn, P. (1973). Concept of social skill with special reference to the behavior of depressed persons. dournal pf Consulhing and Clinical Psycholegy, 69. 304-312. 160 Mahoney, M. J. (1974). 'o a ' ' ° - tion. Cambridge, MA: Billinger. Masters, W., & Johnson, V. (1970). finnen_eexnel_inede; gneey. Boston: Little, Brown. McMullen, R. (1973). The achievement motivation work- shop. Eensennel end Guidance denrnel, pl, 642-645. Meichenbaum, D. (1969). The effects of instructors and reinforcement on thinking and language behaviors of schizophrenics. ehav' ese rch er 101-114. Mendonca, J. D., & Siess, T. F. (1976). Counseling for indecisiveness: Problem-solving and anxiety in man- agement training. Jou n of C unse in Ps c o o 2;, 330-347. Michigan Department of Social Services. Foster Care Serv- ices and Budget Departments. (1989, February). Personal communication. Miller, G. A. (1969). Psychology as a means of promoting human welfare. Anenieen_£eyehelegi__, 2A, 1063- 1075. Millon, T. (1981). 'sord of e sona i ' I Axis l . New York: Wiley. Millon, T. (1982). °1lon Ado esc t so ali v - tony manual. Minneapolis, MN: Interpretive Scoring Systems. Milner, J. &. (1987, March-April). An ecological per- spective on duration of foster care. ghild Welfene, ed, 113-123. Minuchin, S., Chamberlain, P., & Graubard, P. (1967). A project to teach learning skills to disturbed delin- quent children. me ' a J r o o s ' , 21, 558-567. Moon, J. R., & Eisler, R. M. (1983). Anger control: An experimental comparison of three behavioral treat- ments. fiehevien Thenepy, l5, 493-505. Nezu, A. M. (1986). Efficacy of a social problem-solving therapy approach for unipolar depression. gounnel pf gens plhinga and Clinieel Psyehology, EA. 196- -202. 161 Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychonetnic theony. New York: McGraw-Hill. Orlando, N. (1974). The mental patient as therapeutic agent: Self-change, power, and caring. Esyehothen- epy; Thenapy, Reseench and Preehice, ll, 58-62. Pantle, M. L. (1989, April). hssessnent of mood dis- ondens in eldez adelescenhe. Paper presented at the Midwinter Meeting of the Society for Personality Assessment, New York, New York. Pantle, M. L., & Houskamp, J. (1986, March). Ihe_hillen Adoleseent Personality Inventony es a neesnne of sexuel abuse in adolescents. Unpublished research paper presented at the Millon Conference, Miami, FL. Pantle, M. L., & Wassink, K. (1986, March). Qififienenhi; ating suicidal groups with the Millon Adelescenh Personality lnventory. Unpublished research paper presented at the Millon Conference, Miami, FL. Parker, W. L. III. (1987). Effects of e psychoeduca- hi nel intervention on stress, health behavior, end academic achievement in academically underachieving college students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. Parnes, S. J., & Noller, R. B. (1973). Iowand supersen- ihye__§henneled_fineeden. New York: DOK Publishers. Patterson, C., & Carkhuff, R. (1969). Helping end human nelahions: A primer for law and pnofessional help- ers. Vel, 1: Selection and training. Vel, lI: Enaetiee end nesearch. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Patterson, G., & Gullion, M. (1968). Living wihh ehil- dren; New methods :0; panenhs end heeehens. Cham- paign, IL: Research Press. Pierce, R., & Drasgow, J. (1969). Teaching facilitative interpersonal functioning to psychiatric patients. donznel of gennseling Psychology, ld, 295-29 8. Rappaport, A. (1971). The effeets of en intensive eenju- gel neletienship nodifieetion pnognam. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State Univer- sity. 162 Rathus, S. (1973). Instigation of assertive behavior through videotape--Mediated assertive models and direct practice. heheyior Beseeneh and Therapy, ll, 57-65. Richards, C. S., & Perri, M. G. (1978). Do self-control ' treatments last? An evaluation of behavioral problem-solving and faced counselor contact as treat- ment maintenance strategies. ou a o Cou se in Psycholegy, 2;, 376-383. Robin, A. L. (1979). Problem-solving communication training: A behavioral approach to the treatment of parent-adolescent conflict. The_Amenieen_denznel_ef Femily Therapy, 1, 69-82. Robin, A. L. (1980). Parent-adolescent conflict: A skill training approach. In D. P. Rathjen & J. P. Foreyt (Eds.), §ociel eompehence; lntenyentiens fem children and adults. New York: Pergamon. Robin, A. L. (1981). A controlled evaluation of problem- solving communication training with parent-adolescent conflict. Behavior Therapy, l2, 593-609. Robin, A. L., Kent, R. N., O’Leary, K. D., Foster, 8., & Prinz, R. J. (1977). An approach to teaching par- ents and adolescents problem-solving communication skills: A preliminary report. heheyipr_The;epy, 8, 639-643. Rogers, C. (1951). Qlient-cenhened nhezepy. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin. Rubenstein, J. S., Armentrout, J. A., Levin, S., & Harold, D. (1978, October). The parent-therapist program: Alternate care for emotionally disturbed children. Amenican dournel of Qnthopsyehietpy, 66. 654-662. Sanford, F. (1955). Creative health and the principles of habeas mentem. American Psyeholegisn, in, 829- 835. Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1981). Teaching cognitive and social skills to high school students. dennnel e: Cenenlhing and Qlinicel Esyehelogy, 12, 908-918. Seaberg, J. R., & Tolly, E. S. (1986). Predictors of the length of stay in foster care. Social Wonh Beseench and Ahshnaets, 22, 11-l7. 163 Siegel, J. M., 8 Spivack, G. (1976a). Problem-solving therapy: The description of a new program for chronic psychiatric patients. Peyehotherepy; WM. 13. 368-373- Siegel, J. M., 8 Spivack, G. (1976b). A new therapy program for chronic patients. PeheyieL_The;epy, 1, 129-130. Skinner, B. (1938). The hehavior of engenisme. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Skinner. 8- (1953). W. New York: Macmillan. Spivack, G., Platt, J. J., 8 Shure, M. B. (1976). The problem-solving appnoech to adjustmenn. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Straw, M. K., 8 Terre, L. (1983). An evaluation of individualized behavioral obesity treatment and maintenance strategies. Peheyipr_Therepx, 14. 255-266. Timberlake, E. M., 8 Verdieck, M. J. (1987, April). Psychosocial functioning of adolescents in foster care. W. 214-222- Tisdelle, D. A., 8 St. Lawrence, J. S. (1988). Adolescent interpersonal problem-solving skill training: Social validation and generalization. fiehavier Therepy, l2, 171-182. Tracy, H. M. (1986, March). The clinieal pse pf nhe he_T. Unpublished research paper presented at the Millon Conference, Miami, FL. Trenerry, M. R., Pantle, M. L., 8 Ziebelman, K. K. (1988, March). Relationships between the Ronsehech end hhe Millen Adeleseent Personality lnventony. Paper pre- sented at the Midwinter Meeting of the Society for Personality Assessment, New Orleans, LA. VanZytveld, J. R. (1988). do e c s ' '0 end suicide proneness; Gnoup differences meesnned hy shandepdieed neshs. Unpublished research report, Pinerest Christian Hospital, Grand Rapids, MI. 164 Vitalo, R. (1971). Teaching improved interpersonal functioning as a preferred mode of treatment. dounnel e: Qennseling and Qlinieal Psyehology, 2e, 166-171. Watchman, B. (1986, March). The ac erso a it shyle on depnessive subtype . Unpublished research paper presented at the Millon Conference, Miami, FL. Watson, J. (1916). The place of the conditional-reflex in psychology. Psyehology Review, 2;, 89-116. Webb, D. (1988). Specialized foster care as an alterna- tive therapeutic out-of-home placement model. dour- nal pf Qlinieel Child Psyehelogy, 11, 34-43. Weiss, R., Hops, H., 8 Patterson, G. (1973). A framework for conceptualizing marital conflict: A technology for altering it, some data for evaluating it. In L. Hamerlynck, L. Handy, 8 E. Mash (Eds.), Pehavion change: Mehhodology, concepts and practice. Champaign, IL: Research Press. Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibi- hien. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Zupkus, E. C. (1986, March). Millen Adolescent Person- alihy Inventony: Qifferences among normal, emotion- elly disturbed, and delingnenh adolescents. Unpublished research paper presented at the Millon Conference, Miami, FL.