’ ll. [ W.’u|[‘v['5431'[;M[[U.[" [.[[[[[[l[‘"3 3. l :I[;?[['[[[[ 3['[[:T[:;‘ [t “Hi". “3’": ; III-I , . I: ' ' 3:33:15 3331'! l”[“[[ I [ “'3- [:M [l ['1 .3; [ [[‘ 4[[[r[ l'[ I [N ' “ti. [“1 H-\‘[[ 1%.! [H. [321“; “[13’ 3‘3“” ['I'[\ [[ [[U't ([PEH [[ “£9; 41'! I 4% .3." 3333 II;. 3‘3 3.3.3115. 333.:- 1'1.33[[[il"r|3'"3;“th '31“ 3'3”! 0' i [1}. "[ :3[~(i[' 3| [Rh-1 «[II"|! $10330 3'3”. 13:13,!” ”WWW” "' "' T [33" . -.3- [,['_"HI 'J‘H b.2953; . a .._ [315% ,9? [ [II [53% [[[ ' 333,33 53-.3. [,Ldnt ". ‘31}.‘l [:Lv i H: 1,! g. - ’11 [33’ M 'f.‘ ‘ 313s pf. . .3 4:...- ——~ 'fi. ' ‘SMIJ n-..» a.» -V.H.I' «afl- o l -o v -u ‘ .- E. n «3‘ o l v m ‘ - _-e m ’ '. -_r n hi..." .4Io "gm: -. ”If: " ’“C'i: _ - :23 , If.” - .- m .. :0.“ 32333 ~-~3.;I;;6,I. 3.1 i, [31.73 ¢ :I'gxgé an: 3!“ .1. :‘Efilgzgfigit - art.- to! .— 7» .0... ‘3... .921 413.4..- .. .- ‘ _. $3“. - .. -. JIM-CHI: —. - 1". .. - u- o. .‘a a 3... .> .n-v‘ - . M 7; if: I ”’3 :[3 E‘ ‘3 3" l_.'\:‘ ‘33:} 553‘. ”Ifioln. - -- up rug - 88 302 86 5:9- u 84 dig or town) a a a a D D D 0 Days Figure 6. Percentage of Subjects Who Believed the Strategy Helped Improve Performance W Subjects in this study answered open ended questions about how they used imagery. Table 10 is a summary of their responses. 63 Table 10 ResponsesJtheJmaeenLQuestions It helped me concentrate, know what I was planning, it helps to relax, I saw myself and it helped my confidence. The images were clear. Basically I kind of thought with my breathing. When I inhaled it was like I was going back, and when I exhaled it was like I was going forward. That's what I saw and I did that and then visualized it going toward the thing. It was not hard to visualize. Sometimes if I closed my eyes I could see myself do the actual putting. If I didn't it was more of feeling the motion. That seemed to work best, and I could hear it always hitting the little metal thing. So I thought it kind of helped me. It wasn't necessarily seeing it but more of feeling it. The hands went straight back. It works if you concentrate on it. Well when I walked up I actually pictured myself actually playing my friends, pictured myself on the green like when we play for money, and I pictured myself beating them. I could really hear the two sounds like the ball hitting the putter, and the I heard the metal when the ball went into the hole. I wasn't really aware of anything around me and seemed really focused. I saw myself bringing the putter back, not to firm and holding the putter nice and gentle in my hands. If I pushed or pulled the previous one I would always try to see the ball going straight, I could always hear the ball hitting the aluminum cup. I saw myself holding the putter and taking it back and hitting the shot and once I actually did it I thought I could get myself into a zone and just keep doing it even if I missed, I would try to erase this shot and keep trying until I found the perfect shot. I didn't have to do it every time. Doing it once or twice, once if I made it I would keep going like if I made three in a row, but if I missed I would replay it. I mainly concentrated on bringing the putter straight back and hitting the back of the ball. I saw myself taking the backswing hitting the ball, and seeing the ball roll into the hole. The images were vivid, I could see it clear. I saw myself from outside my body. I saw myself putting the ball, more I saw the putter going back and imagined the ball rolling straight toward the target. I didn't see myself, I felt the motion, and I saw the ball and concentrated on the ball. I first imagined what I saw in front of my face, which was that aluminum cup and the ball below me, and the putter looking down but in my mind it was like the putt in progress. The wing contacted the ball and the ball rolling into the cup regardless if I made or missed the shot before. I basically tried to get a feel of how I did it in my mind, and then just let go and did it without thinking. 64 Self-Confidence. Subjects in the study reported feelings of confidence while putting the golf ball. The statement by one subject portrays the image of confidence, “It helped me concentrate, know what I was planning, it helps me to relax, and it helped my confidence.” Another subject said, “I thought I could get myself into a zone, and just keep doing it even if I missed.” Kinesthetic Imagery. Subjects reported using feeling oriented imagery. One subject stated, “Sometimes if I closed my eyes I could see myself do the actual putting. If I didn’t it was more of feeling the motion. That seemed to work best.” Others stated, “ I didn’t see myself, I felt the motion.” “I basically tried to get a feel of how I did it in my mind and then just let go and did it without thinking." Visual Imagery. The subjects reported seeing the ball roll into the hole. For example, one subject stated, “ I could really hear the two sounds like the ball hitting the putter, and I heard the metal when the ball went into the hole.” External Imagery. A few subjects seemed to experience the imagery from a perspective of seeing their own body perform the putting task. This is best exemplified by the following quote, “ I saw myself taking the backswing, hitting the ball, and seeing the ball roll into the hole. The images were vivid, I could see it clear, I saw it from outside my body. Internal Imagery. A few subjects seemed to experience the imagery from a perspective internally, as if they were seeing the putting task as if it were actually happening. This is best exemplified by the following quote, “ I first imagined what I saw in front of my face, which was that aluminum cup and the ball below me, and the putter looking down, but in my mind it was like the putt in progress.” 65 Discussion The main purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness of using a mental preparation strategy (i.e, preperformance positive imagery, preperformance positive affirmations) in improving golf putting performance and accuracy over a no practice control group and practice control group. Additionally, an attempt was made to study the effects of using an educational phase, general phase and specific phase on adherence to using the strategy, and belief in the mental preparation strategy to help improve performance. Results of the study showed that the preperformance imagery group (PI) and the preperformance affirmation group (PA) group did not improve significantly on either of the two measures when compared to a practice control group who was not given a strategy to use. As expected, both mental preparation groups did improve performance significantly when compared to a no practice control group who only putted on the first and last days. In general the hypotheses stated in Chapter I have been partially supported. The discussion section of this chapter will be presented in three sections. The first section will describe the analysis on the performance and accuracy measures for the golf putting task. The second section will analyze and describe the results of the post-experimental questionnaire and the manipulation checks. Finally the third section will present a general discussion of the findings. Wk The first hypotheses in this study predicted that there will be a statistically significant difference between pre and post performance scores for the PI, PA, and the PC groups. After analyzing the results the analysis did not support this hypotheses. Although nonsignificant, the PI group improved from an 66 average of 8.9 putts made to an average of 13.7 putts made on the last day. The PA group improved from an average of 8.9 putts on the first day to an average of 13.4 putts on the last day. These results are not consistent with past research (Caudill & Weinberg, 1983; Weinberg, Smith, Jackson, & Gould, 1984; Weinberg, Jackson, Seaboume, 1985; Wilkes & Summers, 1984; Woolfolk, Parrish, & Murphy, 1985) in which the imagery or positive affirmation group improved their performance from pre to post test. However, these results are in accordance with past research (Epstein, 1980; Weinberg, Gould, Jackson, & Barnes, 1980; Woolfolk, Parrish, Gottsfeld, & Aitken,1985) that has found imagery or affirmations to not lead to statistically significant differences between pre and post performance scores. The data partially supported the hypothesis that there would be a statistically significant difference between the scores of the PI, PA, PC, and NPC groups with the PI, and PA groups having higher post test performance scores than the PC and NPC groups. Findings showed that the PI, and PA groups improved significantly greater than the NPC group, but not better than the PC group. This finding however is not in accordance with the Woolfolk, Parrish, and Murphy (1985) study which found that the mental preparation strategy group improved significantly greater than a practice control group which putted on all six days. However, the finding that the preperformance mental preparation groups did not significantly improve performance as compared to a practice control group in this study do concur with past research (Epstein, 1980; Weinberg, Gould, Jackson, & Barnes, 1980; Woolfolk, Parrish, Gottsfeld, & Aitken,1985) Although the PI and PA groups did not improve performance significantly over the practice control group it should be noted that on average from pre/ post test the imagery group and the 67 affirmation group improved more than the PC group on performance measures. The first two exploratory hypotheses in this study that there will be a statistically significant difference between pre and post accuracy scores for the PI, PA, and the PC groups. After analyzing the results the analysis did not support this hypotheses. The PI group improved an average of 31.4 to an average of 15.4 the last day. The PA group improved from an average of 36.3 the first day to an average of 17.3 on the last day. The data did not support the hypothesis that there will be a statistically significant difference between the accuracy scores of the PI, PA, PC, and NPC groups with the PI group, and PA groups having higher accuracy scores than the PC and NPC groups. Although these hypotheses were not supported at a significant level it should be noted that on average from pre/post test the imagery group and the affirmation group improved more than the PC or NPC on accuracy measures. Subjects using preperformance imagery and preperformance affirmations, although showing the best overall average improvement across the five blocks, did not improve their scores significantly when compared to a practice control group over five days. Explanations are offered to describe this finding by looking at the past results of the studies most similar. The performance and accuracy results of the present study do not exactly replicate the findings of the Woolfolk, Parrish, & Murphy (1985) study that showed a preperformance mental preparation strategy did significantly better than a control group from pre/post test. Although it is difficult to explain why there are discrepancies in the results of the two studies, they both offer minimal support to the belief that a positive image or thought immediately prior to performance is associated with performance enhancement. 68 One possible explanation for the discrepancy in results is that the practice control group improved a great deal more in the present study than the former study. A reason for the improvement of the practice control group may have been that the numbers of trials in the present study were much greater. The present study conducted 40 more trials than the 1985 study which may have allowed the main predictor of self-efficacy in Bandura's theory to have taken place. By having practiced so much, the practice control group may have started to feel more positive and hence through performance accomplishments of repeated practice, improved performance almost as much as the PI, and PA groups. However, failure to directly assess the practice control's possible self-initiated mental preparation in this study was a weakness and should be assessed in future studies of this nature. It was apparent from a few subjects in the practice control group that they were using both positive and negative affirmations. Occasionally, subjects in the practice control group made statements such as, "damn I missed", or "UMMph", or "C'mon I can make this shot." Although there was a failure in this study to assess the self-initiated cognitive techniques employed by the practice control group, this was not the major concern of this investigation. Of major concern was whether subjects in the PI, and PA groups improved performance, and whether they believed the mental preparation strategies helped them improve performance. WWW Many factors have been studied when trying to test if psyching up improves performance or to explain findings. Most studies have found inconsistent results, however elite athletes continue to give reports of using psyching up strategies to enhance performance. Vealey (1986) has suggested that imagery and mental preparation strategies have a better chance to be 69 successful if the subjects or athletes believe that these strategies will help them. In testing this suggestion an educational phase was used based on the suggestions of Vealey (1986). The present study was unique from other psyching up studies in that it included an educational phase for the PI and PA groups to persuade subjects to believe that this strategy could help them improve their performance. Before each experimental session subjects read an informative 2 page persuasive essay suggesting that the mental preparation strategy improves performance. The present investigation found that the subjects reported that they believed in the mental preparation strategies ability to help improve performance. Results of the physical dependent measures support that the contention that belief in mental preparation will be associated with enhanced performance. However, this strategy and belief in the strategy was not helpful in producing significantly higher performance and accuracy scores than the NPC and PC groups. One possible explanation for these findings may have been due to the fact that these techniques were perceived as different and strange. Subjects may have remained skeptical throughout the experiment even though they stated that they believed that these techniques were helpful in improving performance. In addition, subjects did not have a choice of choosing another strategy, even if they felt that it helped improve performance. Subjects were not able to choose another strategy that may have been more effective for them to use based on individual and personal style. One note of importance is the percent of time the subjects used the strategy taught to them. Subjects for the most part used the strategy most of the time. This, indicated that subjects believed that the strategy may have been helping them improve their performance. 70 WW Self-Confidence. Many of authors have suggested that self-confidence is one of the most important aspects of excellence in sport (Feltz, 1988; Highlen & Bennett, 1979, 1983; Mahoney & Avener, 1977). Only a few golfers reported associating the imagery strategy with self-confidence. However, of importance in Bandura’s Self-Efficacy Theory (1977) is whether imagery strategies can vicariously create heightened confidence. Future research should carefully decipher if confidence is heightened by using imagery as a preperformance mental preparation strategy. The results of imagery probe in this study would suggest that imagery may heighten confidence for some subject and not for others. Kinesthetic Imagery. Many of the subjects reported that they used more of a feeling oriented imagery instead of visualizing or seeing anything. The imagery literature documents the benefits of using this type of imagery (Martens, 1987; Orlick, 1990). It is believed that this imagery may be related greatly to the psychoneuromuscular hypothesis. Future studies should ask subjects about feeling oriented imagery so that we can begin to understand the processes of this type of imagery. Positive Imagery. Subjects all seemed to be able to use imagery either kinesthetically or visually. Also, the subjects focus seemed to be on the ball rolling into the hole. This shows that the subjects focused on what they were persuaded to focus on. However, future studies should decipher if the subjects would focus on the ball rolling into the hole if they were not told to do this. Imagery Perspective. Subjects visualized using both perspectives. They seemed to choose a perspective based on personal preference. Studies have been equivocal on whether one perspective is better than another (Murphy & Jowdy, 1992). 71 Qeneraldiscnssion The data from this experiment minimally support the belief that using a mental preparation strategy can improve performance over time. Furthermore, the results support prior lab research (Woolfolk, Parrish, and Murphy, 1985, Wilkes & Summers, 1984) and anecdotal literature that say that positive thinking and positive imagery can help aid in improving closed motor skills. Also, the present research is consistent with Weinberg, Gould, Jackson, & Barnes (1980) who found that mental preparation including imagery and positive affirmation statements did not improve performance of beginning and advanced tennis players significantly over the control condition. A possible explanation to explain why significant results over the practice control group did not occur may be that using mental preparation techniques is task specific and only seems to show significant improvement over a control group in strength tasks (Shelton & Mahoney, 197 8; Weinberg, Gould, & Jackson, 1980). Because the golf putting task is not a strength task and requires accuracy and precision, it may have been difficult to show immediate positive effects of using affirmations and imagery over a practice control group. However using those strategies may have been laying the groundwork for future success. It was thought that five days would be long enough to show an effect for golf putting because of the results of a previous study (Woolfolk, Parrish, & Murphy, 1985). However, affirmations and imagery may work best for causing immediate effects for strength tasks such as those done in recent lab studies (Wilkes & Summers, 1984; Tynnes & McFatter, 1987). Another general possible explanation for not finding performance and accuracy results significantly better than the practice control group could have been that subjects did not have enough practice in using the technique. 72 Weinberg, Gould, Jackson, and Barnes (1980) suggested that practicing a strategy may be more advantageous than just telling subjects to use various cognitive strategies. It could be that for a cognitive strategy to be effective, considerable practice may be needed. Because of this, the present study introduced an educational, general, and specific phase to the experimental group based on the suggestions of previous researchers (Vealey, 1986; Weinberg, Gould, Jackson, & Barnes, 1980). In the present study it was thought that enough education and practice was given to the experimental group since the Woolfolk, Parrish, & Murphy (1985) study did not have these phases, and were still able to show significant improvement. From a psychological standpoint the educational exposure was long enough to persuade subjects to believe the cognitive technique was helpful in improving performance. However, from a physical standpoint more general and specific training may be needed than what was given in the present study. Because all practice groups improved performance over days, instructors and golfers may want to reevaluate strategies used in putting. It seems that subjects similar to the ones in this study can improve performance by utilizing a mental preparation strategy or through continued practice. Typically, instructors say that practice makes perfect and that the traditional thought on how to improve is repetition. However, a new philosophy is to use mental preparation. Although one strategy may not lead to better results in a lab setting over five days, the fact that subjects in the experimental group believed they could make the putt is very important from a practical standpoint. For example, when subjects have a pressure putt, if they use imagery or self-talk it may feel automatic and they may have a better chance than just putting and letting fear disrupt the effortlessness of the putt. 73 Considering the results of this study, using imagery and positive affirmations may not be for everybody. However, we should educate learners about these techniques. Golfers may not need to be told how to think, although the results of the study do not show that it hurts performance. Instructors should present golfers with the mental strategy but not force them to use it. In summary, the data analysis seem to show that preperformance imagery and preperformance affirmations can be considered a useful adjunct to helping improve performance. Also, equally effective was the practice group in which the subjects were not instructed to use a mental practice strategy. Therefore, these techniques should be offered to novices and let them choose the one that they enjoy most. Chapter V Summary, Conclusions, and Suggestions For Future Study The overall purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness of using preperformance mental preparation strategies in order to enhance performance and accuracy over a practice control group and a no practice control group in a closed motor skill. Specifically, the four groups included a no practice control group, a practice control group, a preperformance imagery group and a preperformance positive affirmation group. An experiment using the golf putting task was used. The data helped add to an understanding of the utilization of mental preparation strategies. The present study investigated the effects of using preperformance mental preparation strategies in order to enhance performance and accuracy over a practice control group and a no practice control group in a closed motor skill. Subjects in the study were 40 introductory psychology students who participated in the experiment for course credit. The 40 subjects all performed the basal measure on the golf putting task and were then randomly assigned to one of four groups, a no practice control group (NPC), a practice control group (PI), a preperformance positive affirmation group (PA), and preperformance positive imagery group (PI). The PA and PI groups were then given a group session in which they were shown videos about their strategy, and presented with general exercises introducing them to the strategy they were to use. The PC and NPC were not given a group session. On the second through fifth day of putting the PC group putted twenty shots without being instructed to use a mental preparation strategy. The NPC only putted one more session of twenty putts without being instructed to use a mental preparation strategy. The PI 74 75 and the PA groups, on the second through fifth day of putting were instructed to use their assigned mental preparation strategy. The PI and PA groups following each day filled out manipulation strategy checks to assess how often they used the strategy and whether or not they believed the strategy helped them improve performance. Results of the present study revealed that the PI, PA, PC and NPC, did not improve performance and accuracy from pre to post test at a significant level. Also, the PI, PA, PC were found to be significantly different from the NPC pre to post test in performance measures, but not in accuracy measures. However no differences between the PI, PA, and PC groups were found. Possible explanations for the PI, and PA groups not improving at a statistically significant level over the PC group may be that not enough practice of the mental preparation strategy was given to the PI, and PA, and also maybe mental preparation is most effective when a strength task is used as the task for the dependent measure. Of importance in this study is that the subjects in both experimental groups believed that the mental preparation strategy was helpful in improving performance, possibly indicating that the educational exposure manipulation was successful. Conclusions Based on the findings of this study with regard to the research hypotheses, the following conclusions are made: 1. Preperformance imagery and preperformance affirmations may aid in enhancing the performance of the golf putting task,over a group that does not practice. 2. Practice over five days may aid in enhancing the performance of the golf putting task, over a group that does not practice the task. 76 3. Subjects believed that the mental preparation strategies were helpful in improving golf putting performance. 4. Among the PC, PI, PA groups, all were equally effective in enhancing the performance of the golf putting task. 5. No support was provided to show that mental preparation strategies significantly increased performance and accuracy over a control group that physically practiced the same amount of days. Suggestions For Future Study The findings of the present study seem to warrant these following suggestions for future study. 1. One research direction in this area should probably be to further examine what type of intervention elicits performance improvement over a practice control group. 2. Of interest would be a study in which subjects are screened for both motivational factors and belief in the mental preparation strategy to help improve performance. 3. In order to test if psyching up performance measures are task specific, as this study may suggest, two tasks for each group could be used, to further address this question. 4. A future suggestion in the next study is to have one group read the persuasive literature everyday and have another group using the same intervention not read the persuasive literature. This could possibly test how important it is for subjects to believe in the mental preparation strategy that they are using. It would be hypothesized that the persuasive literature with intervention groups would have a higher belief in the mental preparation strategy due to the mere exposure effect. 77 5. In accordance with many of the researchers in motor learning and sport psychology, the next step in this type of research is to use athletes who are participating in junior golf, high school golf, or college golf in an applied field setting. Therefore, researchers would begin to understand how using these techniques affect advanced, motivated golfers. Questions that could be considered are would the strategy disrupt the normal routine of an advanced player? Would these players find these strategies as helpful as those in this study who were less advanced players. 6. Finally, what is of major need is a longitudinal study in which mental preparation strategies are used to determine psychological and physical effects. A longer study will allow for teaching and learning of the strategy in great detail. 78 Appendix A Informed Consent I give my informed consent to participate in this study of how psychological techniques influence sport performance. I agree to the publication of the results of this study as long as the information is kept confidential so that no individual identification can be made. I also understand that a record will be kept of my having participated in the experiment, but my individual data of having participated will only be able to be identified by number only. (1) I have been informed that my participation in this experiment will involve using a psychological process before golf putting. (2) I have been informed that the general purpose of this experiment is to investigate how participants use psychological techniques in performing sport skills. (3) I have been informed that there are no known risks or discomforts involved in my participation in this experiment. (4) I have been told that there are no "disguised" procedures in this experiment. Procedures in this study can be taken at face value. (5) I have been informed that the investigator will take the time to answer any questions about the procedures, at any point in the testing session. (6) I have been informed that at any time I am free to discontinue my participation at any time during the experiment. If you have any concerns about any procedures or aspects of the study, you are referred to the UCRIHS Committee, 232 Administration Building, Michigan State University. (experimenter signature) (participant signature and date) Directions: Please complete the following background questions to 79 Appendix B Background Questionnaire the best of your ability: 1. Age: 2. Sex: m f 3. Please circle the following statement which best describes your golf putting ability: novice intermediate advanced 4. Have you ever used or been taught any mental preparation techniques to improve sports performance. Yes No 5. Have you taken golf classes at MSU? __Yes NO If Yes, beginner __ intermediate 6. Do you own your own golf clubs? Yes NO 7. Did you play on a golf team in high school? Yes NO If yes How many years _—., avg. score, Handicap 8. Have you played in golf tournaments Yes NO 9. Have you had private golf instruction lessons from a Professional? Yes No Thank you for completing the questionnaire. 80 Appendix C I i . I I II E . . | What is imagery? Imagery is using your imagination to rehearse something in your mind. For example, you may have had the experience of making an important phone call that you were scared to make. Before you actually made the phone call you rehearsed what you were going to say. Another imagery experience may have been when someone asked you for directions. When giving the directions you may have imagined yourself driving the route. Or before putting a golf ball you may have imagined a putt falling in to the hole. Imagery is rehearsal in your mind of a skill you are about to perform. You may be asking well then how do we know that imagery works. The evidence is outstanding. Great athletes speak of using imagery to obtain optimal levels of performance. Dwight Stones a three time Olympian high jumper used imagery before every jump. Many other athletes have used imagery in athletics. Such famous athletes as Fran Tarkenton, Chris Evert, Dick Fosburry, and O.J. Simpson all say they have used imagery to improve their performance. You may be wondering if any famous golfers have used the skill of imagery. The answer is clearly yes. Many of today's top professionals use this technique to obtain great performance. Jack Nicklaus is probably the most famous athlete to attest to using imagery before every shot he ever hit in golf. He described imagery as going to the movies before every shot. Nicklaus says that before every shot he sees were the ball is to finish, secondly he sees the ball roll or fly to the intended target, and finally, he imagines the swing that will get him the intended results. You are probably wondering how we know that imagery works. Imagery works because your muscles cannot distinguish between something that actually occurred and something that is imagined. 81 Therefore, if you imagine putting the golf ball before putting you are actually building the correct mental machine to perform the skill. As a result of imaging the putt, our strokes become more fluid and automatic. WWW Golfers of all ability should use imagery before every putt for three reasons (1) Imagery builds a mental machine and helps the stoke become fluid and automatic, (2) Imagery improves concentration (by visualizing yourself making a putt prior to attempting it). You are in a sense stopping your mind from wandering and thinking irrelevant thoughts, (3) Using imagery before putting builds self—confidence in your putting stroke. You must remember that imagery is not magic. It is a skill that must be practiced to be effective. Using imagery before every putt will give you a better chance to make each putt. 82 Appendix D W It is important to understand the formation of using basic images before using imagery in sport. There are two components of imagery we are now going to practice. The first component is Vividness, and the second component is controllability. W The following exercise is taken from Vealey (1986). Exercise 1 Pick a close friend or someone that you are around quite often. Have the person sit in a chair in front of you. Try to get a sharp image of the person. Try to visualize the details of the person: facial features, body build, mannerisms, clothes, etc. Now imagine that person talking. Still focusing on the person's face, try to hear his or hear voice. Imagine all of the person's facial expressions as he or she talks. Think about how you feel about the person. Try to recreate the emotions you feel toward him or her whether they be warm friendship, deep love, or admiration and respect. The following exercises were taken from Bump (1989). Exercise two "Think back to the bedroom you had when you were 12 years old. Do you know what it looked like. Create a picture in your mind's eye of that bedroom. You are standing in the doorway looking in. Notice the color of the walls...and see the floor. Focus on the texture and color of the floor covering. Now step into your room and look around. See your dresser, you desk, the chair, and the windows; see the pictures and all of your favorite things. Now focus on your 83 bed. Notice the wood finish. The look at the bed spread; notice its color, its texture, the way it hangs on the bed. in your minds eye reach out and touch the bedspread. Is it smooth or knobby, fluffy or flat? now step back and take one last look around. The windows are open and a cool breeze brings the outdoor smells to your attention. Focus on those smells. Hear the sounds of your family and friends gathering. Now, focus on the feelings you had when you lived in that room."(Bump, 1989, p.100). imagemfinntmflabilitxfixnosunc The following exercises will be practices as training for controllability Exercise 3 "The exercise begins with a good friend. Try to create a truly vivid image of this friend. Now imagine that you have sprinkled that friend with "Ratio" a magical potion that allows you to change his or her size. Gradually shrink the person you have selected down to the size of a soda can. Try to see a gradual change. After you shrink your subject, return him or her to normal size. Then go the other way. Make your friend into a giant. Before you finish, always return your friend to his or her normal size."(Bump, 1989, p. 100) Exercise 4 "Imagine that you are holding a bucket in your right hand and extend your arm straight out to the side at shoulder level. Feel the weight of the bucket as you hold it straight out to the side. Now imagine someone pouring 5 pounds of sand into the bucket. Feel how heavy your arm begins to feel. Focus on how much heavier the bucket feels as an additional five pounds of sand is added. Let yourself feel the heaviness of your bucket as your arm gets more and more tired. Feel your arm getting heavier and heavier... very, very heavy. Focus on the heaviness of your arm. Now someone takes the heavy bucket from you. Tell yourself that your hand and arm feel pretty normal again. Let 84 your hand and arm come back down to your side and relax." (Bump, 1989, p. 100) Now some more general imagery exercises will be given: The following exercises were taken from Harris & Harris (1984). Exercise 5 "In your mind's eye visualize a spot of color such as red, zoom in until your whole visual field is covered with red, zoom out again until it recedes into blackness. Change the color to blue and repeat, continue this exercise using several colors. Each time start with a small dot of color, let it approach you like an oncoming light until it engulfs your entire visual field, then reverse direction until it disappears."(p. 110). Exercise 6 "In your mind's eye pick up a lemon. Feel the texture of the oiliness. Smell the lemon, roll it on the table, take a bite out of the end and taste the juice. Note all the ways you can experience a lemon in your imagination"(p. 111). 85 Appendix E W The imagery group will receive the following exposure session similar to Epstein (1980). This following will be read to each subject. I will now ask you to form images that are related to the task of golf putting. As previously indicated this technique is called mental imagery, and it has been used by great athletes such as Jack Nicklaus, Fran Tarkenton, and Greg Lougainis, to enhance performance in sport skills. First I will describe to you a scene, and I want you to try and imagine it as clearly as possible. If you are experiencing trouble in imaging the description, relax, and remember it is time to become familiar with the task. When imaging try to stay inside your body as if you are actually performing the putting technique. Try and see things through your own eyes. Try to pretend you are making the putt. It is only important now that you understand that your goal is to try and mentally image as clearly as possible. Now I want you to imagine seeing yourself walking up to the golf ball. Try and see the line of the putt. Try to visualize yourself holding the putter in your hand. Now visualize making a gentle but firm backswing and stroking the putt smoothly down the target line seeing the ball rolling, rolling right into the cup. Let's put the golf ball again. Visualize another golf ball lying on the putting surface below you. This time really concentrate on taking the club straight back and try to see the ball rolling, rolling straight into the cup. Now let's do it in slow motion. Visualize the ball before you. This time really concentrate on seeing the ball rolling, rolling right into the cup. For about 5 or 10 more times I would like you to practice in your mind seeing the ball and then taking the putter back and striking the back of the ball, as the ball is rolling, rolling, right into the hole. While you are practicing really focus on seeing the ball roll into the hole. Practice as many putts in your 86 mind as you want but remember to try and see the ball rolling, rolling into the hole. Like many other sports, imaging the golf putt in your mind will become easier with time. Go ahead and start practicing putting until I tell you to stop. 87 Appendix F Affirmations are what you say to yourself. For example have you ever gone into a test knowing that you were not prepared. Chances are you may have said to yourself, "I think I am not going to do very well." This statement is a negative affirmation. Or maybe you have had the experience where you knew everything you were going to do would turn out well. Maybe you knew you were going to get an A on the test, So before you took the test you said to yourself, " I think I am going to do well on the test." This is an example of a positive affirmation. Maybe a sport specific example would be more interesting. Say the Spartans are in the NCAA finals with the score tied and the Spartans worst free throw shooter is on the line. Do you think he should say to himself, "I can make it" before he shoots? Many basketball players and golfers would tell you that the most important thing in shooting a free throw or making a golf putt is to believe you are going to make it. If you can make one then you must have the skill to put it in the hole. One athlete that would agree with using positive affirmations is David Graham. He always believes he can make the putt. Before hitting a shot, the golfer who holds the all time low record for a round in golf, Chip Beck says to himself, "Just Perfect." He then knows he has given the shot the best possible chance of going in. Pro golfer Jeff Thomsen says before every shot, "I can make this shot." And Peter Jacobsen says before every shot, "Knock it close." It is important to use Positive statements so that no negative thoughts enter your mind. Like if you say to yourself, "I hope I do not miss, You are really only hearing I hope I miss. For example, Don't be aware of your breathing? Pause. Most people immediately become aware of their breathing. So if a golfer makes a negative statement before putting. The negative statement usually comes true. There 88 are two main reasons why you should use positive affirmations: (1) When you say a positive affirmation to yourself it improves concentration to focus on the task at hand. (2) Your confidence will be heightened which may lead to an automatic stroke. It is important to remember to only say positive affirmations before the skill. Remember, it is best when actually putting to have no thoughts in your mind. 89 Appendix G Basic Affirmation Exposures (PA) Now we will go over some basic affirmation exercises. I would like you to now read the following affirmations to yourself. Please read each one ten times. Every day in every way I am greater and greater I like who I am I trust myself I am relaxed I forgive my errors I can do it I enjoy life I am on my side I always do the best job I can I am proud of my efforts I can do anything I choose to do Basic affirmations can make you feel good about who you are. The second exercise we are going to do is to fill in these statements: Iamagood I am an efficient I am a creative 90 I am a relaxed I am a courageous I am a strong I am a purposeful I am a graceful The third exercise is an activity about a golfer who is trying out for a the Spartan Golf team. George is a very talented golfer. Over the course of his career he has been quite successful, compiling above average scores. As a high schooler he was awesome. Now he is trying to make the Michigan State University Golf-team. He is not on a scholarship. As George got to the West course for his first day of tryouts, the senior and junior golfers hung out together, while George was alone waiting for his name to be called so he could tee off. While practicing on the putting green George began to question his ability to make the team. While warming up he continually saw the members of the golf team making almost every putt they attempted. Then a little while later at the driving range, George began to dwell on how far and accurate the members of the golf team hit the ball. George began to think to himself, " I can't possibly beat these guys." As you probably would guess this thought made things worse. Then the following thoughts passed through his mind What will my parents and friends think of me if I lose What if I can't hit the ball straight What if I can't make any putts What if I slice the first shot in the water. 91 Because these thoughts took over George's mind, he was not at all ready for the competition. He then heard his name called to tee off, and got an uneasy feeling in his stomach. Now answer the following questions: 1. What was the causes of George's nervousness? 2. What negative statements may have George been making to himself? 3. What could George say to himself to focus on the present and not worry about others? 92 Appendix H W I will now ask you to tell yourself you are a good putter. Positively reaffirm to yourself that you are a good putter and that you can be successful at this task. Also, tell yourself that if you do not make the putt, that does not mean that you were not successful. You will be able to putt again shortly. I am asking you to think positive about golf putting because professional and low handicap golfers allude to becoming great putters by thinking positively. They often say that anyone can putt with any technique, but the putter's who think they can make the putt are usually the best. Also, there once was a legendary former Oklahoma State University golf coach who was one of the best putters. However, his technique was not that impressive. There was nothing special about what he did. Gary Wiren a former golf professional and sport psychologist Richard Coop state, " It was his attitude, not his method, that let him sink so many putts. His faith in himself was total. He said he was a great putter, he believed it. And he was. At this time I would like you to repeat these statements to yourself. "I am a good putter, (pause), I am a good putter,", I am a successful person". Now go ahead for the next minute and tell yourself that you are a good putter. 93 Post Experimental (haestionnaire (Appendix I) Directions : Please answer the questions to the best of your ability. In the following questions, please circle the appropriate number or answer corresponding to the question asked. 1. What percentage of the time did you use the mental preparation strategy presented to you? (Please estimate) 0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100% 2. Do you believe the mental preparation strategy taught to you was helpful in improving performance? Yes No 3. Would you consider using this strategy in other sports or facets of life? Yes No Please elaborate: 4. When you missed a putt how often did you think you would not make the next putt because you missed the putt directly before? never sometimes often always 5. Did missing a putt make you try harder to make the next putt? Yes No 6. When you missed a putt how often did you think that the mental preparation routine taught to you was not working? never sometimes often always Thank you for completing the questionnaire. 94 Appendix J Debriefing The purpose of this investigation was to increase understanding about using mental preparation immediately before performing a motor skill. This investigation tested the hypothesis that using psychological processes directly before performance will increase performance over a control group. Four groups participated in the experiment. In one group subjects were asked to perform the skill in their mind directly before executing the skill. In a second group participants were asked to say to themselves before putting, I think I can make this putt" directly before performance. The control groups were only instructed to try and sink each putt. It is expected that the two experimental groups will improve performance over the control groups. If you have any questions, please do not hesitate to ask the investigator. If you would like a copy of the summary of results of this experiment when completed, please leave your name and address with the experimenter. Thanks for participating in this experiment. 95 Appendix K Means and Standard Deviations for Performance and Accuracy for All Days Table 11 u .0 .0 .0 0| 0' 0 0" 0 00" D "000:... - day1(pre) day 2 day 3 day 4 day 5(post) Means 8.9 11.3 10.6 13.2 13.7 Std dev. 1.91 2.7 3.65 3.19 2.627 Table 12 u .0 00 .,u0._0l' 00 0 0 ’. o 0. I "'00"... ' day1(pre) day 2 day3 day 4 day 5(post) Means 8.9 11.3 11.1 13.1 13.4 Std dev. 2.183 2.710 2.6 2.56 2.413 Table 13 96 u (0 .0 .0 0| 0. I’k 0 0. D "Icon... ’ day1(pre) day 2 day3 day 4 day 5(post) Means 10.1 9.9 11.6 12.9 13.4 Std dev. 2.08 3.75 2.17 2.85 2.413 Table 14 u ._. .0 .0._0l' 0. .‘s' 0 001 "00ou.,u ‘ Day1(pre) Day 2(post) Means 8.2 9.0 Std dev. 1.874 3.204 Table 15 97 U a. 10 10 0. 00 0 0" o 08" I. gqng day1(pre) day 2 day3 day 4 day 5(post) Means 31.4 24.0 28.6 16.2 15.4 Std dev. 9.38 7.61 11.52 6.877 6.518 Table 16 u . . . 0 . 0 0 D 0 . 0 . ’ . 0 0 O l . q q day1(pre) day 2 day3 day 4 day 5(post) Means 36.3 25.5 25.6 22.2 17.3 Std dev. 9.38 7.66 6.293 8.364 8.512 98 Table 17 U I u 0 o 0 0 I 1 ' o 1 o u a o 0 I I . day1(pre) day 2 day3 day 4 day 5(post) Means 30.0 29.3 25.2 19.7 19.1 Stddev. 12.02 14.56 10.61 8.945 10.214 LIST OF REFERENCES Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change W 8.4. 191- 215 Bird, E. I. (1984). EMG quantification of mental practice. W Skills, 52, 899-906. Brody, E. B. Hatfield, B. D., & Spalding, T. W. (1988). Generalization of self- efficacy to a continuum of stressors upon mastery of a high-risk sport skill W19, 32- 34 Boutcher, S. H., & Rotella, R. J. (1987). A psychological skills educational program for closed-skill performance enhancement. W Esxchologist. 1, 127-137. Bump, L. (1989). Wide, Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. : Champaign, Illinois. Bunker, L., & Williams, J. M. (1986). Cognitive techniques for improving performance and building confidence. In Jean M. Williams (ed.): Mayfield Publishing Company: Palo, Alto California. Caudill, D. & Weinberg, R. S. (1983). The effects of varying the length of the psych-up interval on motor performance. War, 5. 231-235. Caudill, D. , Weinberg, R., & Jackson. A. (1983). Psyching-up and track athletes: A preliminary investigation. EDIE—01W 5, 2 31-2 3 5. Clark, L. V. (1960). Effect of mental practice on the development of a certain motor skill. WELL 31, 560-569. 99 100 Corbin, C. B. (1967). Effects of mental practice on skill development after controlled practice. Researchfluanexm, 31, $60-$69. Corbin, C. B. (1972). Mental practice. In W. P. Morgan (Ed.), W musculauzerformance.(pp. 94-118). New York: Academic Press. Corbin, C. B., Laurie, D. R., Gruger, C., & Smiley, B. (1984). Vicarious success experience as a factor influencing self-confidence, attitudes, and physical activity of adult women lournalcfleachinzjnlmcicaL Education .4. 17- 23 Egstrom, G. H. (1964). Effects of an emphasis on conceptualizing techniques during early learning of a gross motor skill. W 35. 472-481. Epstein, M. L. (1980). The relationships of mental imagery and mental practice to performance of a motor task. WM 2, 2 1 1-2 20. Feltz, D. L. (1988). Self-confidence and sports performance. W Snortsjciencefleadms. 1.6, 423 -.458 Feltz, D. L. (1992). The Nature of Sport Psychology. InT. S. Horn (Ed. ), WW®D 3-11). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Feltz, D. L. & Landers, D. M. (1983). The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance:A meta-analysis. 19mm Emnolozx. 5. 25-57. Feltz, D. L., Landers, D. M., & Raeder, U. (1979). Enhancing self-efficacy in high avoidance motor tasks: A comparison of modeling techniques. loumalciSoortEmholosLJ. 112-122. Gould, D. (1990). AAASP: A vision for the 1990's. WM Esxcholozx. P. 99-1 16- Gould, D., & Weiss, M. (1981). Effect of model similarity and model self-talk on self-efficacy in muscular endurance. MW 3., 1 7-2 9. 101 Gould, D., Weiss, M., & Weinberg, R. S., (1981). Psychological characteristics of successful and nonsuccessful Big Ten wrestlers. Whimsy. 3, 69-81. Gould, D., Weinberg, R. S., & Jackson, A. (1980). Mental preparation strategies , cognitions, and strength performance. 19112113115203. Euchoiozx. L 329-339. Graham, D. (1990). MentaLlloughnessIraininefoLGolf. New York: The Stephen Greene Press/Pellham Books. Greenspan, M. J., & Feltz, D. L. (1989). Psychological interventions with athletes in competitive situations: A review. W 3, 2 1 9-2 36. Hale, B. D. (1982). The effects of internal and external imagery on muscular and ocular concomitants. MW, 4. 379-387. Hall, C, Buckoltz., & Fishburne, G. J. (1992). Imagery and the acquisition of motor skills CanadianJournalcf AooliedSoort.Scienco. 11. 19-27 Harris D V., &Harris B L (1984) WWW MentaLSkillefoLEhxsicaLEeonle New York: Leisure Press Hecker, J. E. & Kaczor, L. M. (1988). Application of imagery theory to sport psychology: Some preliminary findings. 11538211919231, 1.9., 363- 373. Highlen, P. S., & Bennett, B. B. (1979). Psychological characteristics of successful and nonsuccessful elite wrestlers: An exploratory study. loumachSnotflsxcholosx, l. 123-137. Highlen, P. S., & Bennett, B. B. (1983). Elite divers and wrestlers: A comparison between open-and closed-skilled athletes. WW 5, 390-409. Hinshaw, K. E. (1991). The effects of mental practice on motor skill performance. Critical evaluation and meta-analysis. Imaginatm Wild-.35 Hogan, P. I., & Santomeier, J. P. (1984). Effects of mastering swim skills on older adultS' self-efficacy. Researchiluattctlufor—Excrciseaudm 56,.284—2 96. 102 Jacobson, E. (1932). Electrophysiology of mental activities. mm mm 4.4. 677-694. Kelsey, l. B. (1961). Effects of mental and physical practice upon muscular endurance. Researchfluarterlx. 32. 47-54. Lutkus, A. D. (1975). The effect of "imaging" on mirror drawing. W thefsuchonomicSocictx. 5. 389- 390. McFadden, S. (1982). The relative effectiveness of two type of imagery rehearsal applied as psych-up strategies to improve skilled goaltending performance. In Salmela, J. Orlick, T., and Partington, J.(ed.). NEAL W Mahoney, M. J., & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the elite athlete. An exploratory study W 2 135-141 Maltz, M. (1960). W. New York, Prentice Hall. Marks, D. (1977). Imagery and consciousness: a theoretical review from an individual differences perspective. JoumalgLMentaUmagem, 1, 275- 290. Martens, R. (1987). WWW- Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Mcauley, E. (1992).Se1f-referent thought in sport and physical activity. (on 101- -118) MT S Horn (ed) Wham (pp 3- 11). Champaign IL: Human Kinetics. Mendoza, D., & Wichman, H. (1978). "Inner" darts: effects of mental practice on performance of dart throwing. W 51. 1195- 1 199. Meyers, A. W., Cooke, C. J., Cullen, J. & Lilles, L. (1979). Psychological aspects of athletic competitors: A replication across sports. WW Research. A 361 -366 Murphy, 5. M. (1990). Models of imagery in sport psychology: A review. JournaloLMentaUmagem 1:1, 15 3-172. 103 Murphy, S. M. & Jowdy D. P. (1992). Imagery and mental practice. In T. S. Horn (ed.). WW (pp-221250). Champaign. IL: Human Kinetics. Murphy, S. M. & Woolfolk, R. L. (1987). The effects of cognitive interventions on competitive anxiety and performance on a fine motor skill accuracy task mtematmnalJoumachSoonPsychomlfit 152- 166 Mumford, P, & Hall, C. (1985). The effects of internal and external imagery on performing figures in figure skating CanadianJonrnanLAoplied. SoonSciences. 1.0. 171- 177 Nelson, L. R. & Furst, M. L. (1972). An objective study of the effects of expectation on competitive performance. MW 69- 72. Ness, R. G., & Patton, R. W. (1979). The effects of beliefs on maximum weight lifting performance. WW3. 205- 211. Nicklaus, J. (1974). W. New York: Simon and Schuster. Orlick, T. (1990). Wan ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Oslin, J. L. (1985). A meta-analysis of mental practice research:Differentiation between intent and type of cognitive activity utilized. Unpublished master's thesis, Kent State University Kent, OH. Pavio, A. (1985). Cognitive and motivational functions of 1magery in human performance W. 1.0. 22- 28 Perry, H. M. (1939). The Relative efficiency of actual and imaginary practice in five selected tasks._Ar_cm¥es_Q££sychglogy, BA, 5-75. Porter. K.. & Foster. 1. (1986). IbementaLathletennncrttaminafoLoeak W2. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown. Richardson, A. (1967). Mental practice: A review and discussion (Part 1) W38. 95- 107 Richardson, A. (1969). W. New York: Springer. 104 Ross, 5. L. (1985). The effectiveness of mental practice in improving the performance of college trombonists. WM; Education..3.3. 231-230. Rotella, R. J., Gansender, B., Ojala, D., & Billing, J. (1980). Cognitions and coping strategies of elite skiers: An exploratory study of young developing athletes. WW 2, 350-354. Rushall, B. S. (1984). The content of competition thinking. In W. F. Straub & J. M. Williams (Eds). Watchdog): (pp. 51-62). Lansing. NY: Sport Science Associates. Ryan, E. D., & Simons, J. (1981). Cognitive demand imagery and frequency of mental practice as factors influencing the acquisition of mental skills. loumalcffioottfimhology. 4. 35-45. Sackett, R. S. (1934). The Influences of symbolic rehearsal upon the retention of a maze habit. lonmaioLGcneraLEmholoum. 376-395. Shelton, T. O., & Mahoney, M. J. (1978). The content and effect of "psyching- up" strategies in weight lifters. WW 2. 275-2 84. Shick, J. (1970). Effects of mental practice on selected volleyball skills for college women. Researchfluarterly. .41. 88-94. Smyth, M. M. (1975). The role of mental practice in skill acquisition. Journal oLMotoLBchaidor. 1. 199-206. Start, K. B. (1962). The influence of subjectively assessed "games ability" on gain in motor performance after mental practice on the acquisition of a motor skill. imamaloLGcncrallsychoiou. 61. 169-173. Stebbins, R. J. (1968). A comparison of the effects of mental practice in learning a motor skill. W, 32, 714-720. Steel, W. I. (1952). The effect of mental practice on the acquisition of a motor skill loumaloLEhysicaLEducationfld. 101- 208 105 Suinn, R. M. (1980). Psychology and sports performance: Principles and applications In R Suinn (ed..) Wand. applications (pp. 26- -36.) Minneapolis: Burgess. Winning, W. E. (1949). Mental practice and physical practice in learning a motor skill. W 2.0.. 432-435. Tynes, L. L., & McFatter, R. M. (1987). The efficacy of "psyching" strategies on a weightlifting task WWII. ll. 327- 336 Ungerleider. 8.. &Golding.J M.. (1992) BeyondStLengthsfisychplogical. W William C. Brown Publishers: Dubuque, Iowa. Van Noord, N. L. (1984). Development and evaluation of a self-talk assessment instrument for tennis players. Unpublished Dissertation, Michigan State University, E. Lansing, Michigan. Vandel, R. S., Davis, R. D., & Clugston, H. A. (1943). The function of mental practice in the acquisition of motor skills. W Psychology . 22. 243-250. Vealey, R. S. (1986). Imagery training for performance enhancement. In Jean M Williams (ed) Whip. W Palo Alto, California: Mayfield Publishing Company. Washburn, M. F. (1916). W. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Waterland. J C (1956). Iheeffectofmentalptacticecomhinodmth l’ I’ ‘I '8‘. III’Q ’I ’._.I 'I'IIIIIII. W. Unpublished masters dissertation, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI. Weinberg, R. S. (1981). The relationship between mental preparation strategies and motor performance: A review and critique. Quest, 33,, 195-2 13. Weinberg, R. S. (1984). Mental preparation strategies. In J. M. Silva & R. S. Weinberg (eds..) Euchoipgicalfoundationaotsoommp 145- 156) Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. 106 Weinberg, R. S. (1986). Relationship between self-efficacy and cognitive strategies in enhancing endurance performance. IntematignaL WWII. 280-292. Weinberg, R. S., Gould, D., & Jackson, A. (1979). Expectations and performance: An empirical test of Bandura's self-efficacy theory. 1911mm Esychlogxcl. 320-331. Weinberg, R. S. Gould, D., &Jackson, A. (1980). Cognition and motor performance: Effect of psyching-up strategies on three motor tasks. 4, 239-245. Weinberg, R. S., Gould, D., & Jackson, A. (1981). Relationship between the duration of the psych-up interval and strength performance. W W. 3. 166-170. Weinberg, R. S., Gould, D., Jackson, A. & Barnes, P. (1980). Influence of cognitive strategies on tennis serves of players of high and low ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 663-666. Weinberg, R. S., Jackson, A., & Seaboume, T. (1985). The effects of specific vs. nonspecific mental preparation strategies on strength and endurance performance. loumachSmchhaidot. 8. 175-180. Weinberg, R. S., Smith, J., Jackson, A., Gould, D. (1984). Effect of association, dissociation and positive self-talk strategies on endurance performance. WEBB; Whelan, J. P. ,Mahoney, M. J. Meyers, A. W. (1991). Performance enhancement in sport. a cognitive behavioral domain. Wm, 22, 307- 327. Wilkes, R. L., & Summers, J. J. (1984). Cognitions, mediating variables, and strength performance. loumaloffiponisychology. a. 351-359. Wiren, G., Coop, R., & Sheehan, L. (1979). W. New York: Simon & Schuster, Inc. Wrisberg, C. A., & Ragsdale, M. R. (1979). Cognitive demand and practice level: factors in the mental rehearsal of motor skills. Jgumalpfflnmm W. 5. 201-208. 107 Wiggins, D. K, (1984). The History of Sport Psychology in North America. In J. M Silva & R S Weinberg (eds..) EsxchoipeicaLfoundatipnsofjoort.(pp 3- -1.4) Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Woolfolk, R. 1. Murphy, S. M., Gottsfeld, D., & Aitken, D. (1985). The effects of mental practice of task and mental depiction of task outcome on motor performance. lonmaloLSoonmnplou. 7. 191-197. Woolfolk, R., Parrish, W, & Murphy, S. M. (1985). The effects of positive and negative imagery on motor skill performance. 90W Research. 9. 335- 341. ”711111111111lilies