“450' mu: no? "19' 1-3: A4 nu in. ~. ‘4“‘6’L‘ 1U“: gill‘m». hm ~. .~ ”1 u... . £2“ "a: ‘ 0‘s?" . 135. 45;; m ..... A . . I: :3559‘; ".- 3.1: fi;:.?l‘~{“"~: .. (1 Tf'N‘ .‘ ’. Ft}: ‘c‘;§‘.s‘.-u 3.5;” , . 1’...‘ ’14; a" .u' - d. 1': $139: 9’ i, ii ”iii " .. fifimfd" ml W‘T D . i‘ .1 "’fnfvr U: y :‘f ‘1'}: 'l A S V “A 4 ,l. ", l . ' E. ‘1 “'4"; . . - 36’ , , gzézaé .3 * 1. w ‘ "ft-"{5}; ‘3.- '. u :1. Kit Jug: r. ..i 5‘. a. Hug-J". ‘33.. sA‘L ‘x‘JLak' $13." u‘v. 5.1%.... a}? $9.1M .4- _ "3;" J}? ‘ l m 1‘9 ‘~ . ". mu wiicivfl ‘ ’,;~f~”§$}.:'7"?”';§95‘£§— * ‘ ‘ 3‘ . "r. . fl unwrig'fi ... . .-.~:.~.~. ~3er 1' 'c. .151; ‘3‘ .9... ‘n‘. “ L .- w YE}; 2. .5 guégg; Jud?“ Myflmw ‘ r m . 9,11%. X‘J‘e‘ié‘: ‘T 1‘). a. i“:- ‘1? ":‘I “ -\ “a. .ugnflé’ 31" 335-. " l‘ 1421' .. "’1" “A o - number of confidants > number of phone calls per year > number of visits to and from individuals in the social network per year > total number of persons in the network > physical distance from individuals in the network. 17 l 8 In addition, gender differences in type of confidant is hypothesized; that is, when asked to name one confidant, females and males will differ in whom they believe is someone they can talk to about their troubles or concerns: FEMALES: Friends > Spouse > Siblings > Children MALES: Spouse > Children > Siblings > Friends Finally, Table 1 lists the expected significant correlations between such variables as psychological well-being, social support, and perception of health. 19 .32 8. @ Became u .. coma—9:00 PEN u c coca—3.80 95392 n - coma—8.80 Esteem u + *+ *+ 333 6.5 *+ e+ *+ o t... .mmsm Etom mmD .3:an .3500 to: 508m: fog—oz e... e... o e+ e... o c- - o + + o e- e- c c+ o e+ + o + dogma 5.3m 5:82 323. Baum meshes: .. “83205me e + «comm—3m 38m + swam: 5:333: .. c2655 E252 + $529530 foam—2 .. acumeoumm 53mm .- swam sacs. .. con—833m .835 we ewe: scram «o mw< $50.4. Bm< mmDAO mow 2H Damn—Jam; Amm OZ< mUZmHmn—EOU AgOgémm .mBA m0 §~mUMmm mO ZOPSmMy—Oumm—E m0 20:80.53 a mam—<8 METHODS W One-hundred sixty-five participants aged 55 and older were recruited from senior citizen groups, senior nutrition sites, plus retired faculty and staff from Michigan State University (MSU). Phone calls and subsequent site visits were made to the senior citizen groups and nutrition sites. A brief 20 minute explanation of the project was presented and individuals were invited to sign up for a structured interview, memory test battery, and mental health assessment inventories. Follow-up phone calls were made to schedule actual appointments. In lieu of signing up after the presentation, individuals were given the option of contacting the coordinator by phone and scheduling an appointment at that time. Retired staff and faculty from MSU were sent flyers describing the project along with a cover letter which instructed them to call the MSU Psychological Clinic to arrange appointments if they were interested in participating in the research (see Appendix A). Individuals who agreed to participate either came to the MSU Psychological Clinic or a home visit was arranged if travel to the university was either not possible or not desirable. Interviews and tests were administered by trained undergraduate and graduate students from the Department of Psychology at MSU. All interviews were tape-recorded and all participants signed a permission form which gave them the option to drop out of the research at any time (see Appendix B). The entire structured interview, memory tests, and mental health assessment inventories took approximately 1-1 1 / 2 hours to administer. 20 21 In addition to the above, feedback either in person or by telephone was given to every participant in the research. This feedback was designed to give information regarding performance on the tests of memory function; however, if significant depression or other mental health concerns were found, this was also conveyed to the individual along with proper counseling referrals. If memory tests indicated a possible problem, participants were advised to seek medical attention and/ or were asked to come back for retesting in 8 monthsto 1 year. Measures 1) MW (MAI) (Lawton, M. P., Moss, M., Fulcomer, M., Kleban, M. H., 1982). The MAI is an assessment instrument capable of measuring the well-being of the aged in the areas of behavioral competence (health, cognition, time use, social interaction, etc), psychological well-being, and perceived environmental quality. The performance of 590 older people in groups composed of community residents, in—home service clients, and people awaiting admission to an institution was determined. All respondents were interviewed in their homes. Interviewers received three days of intensive training. W (of summary rating scales/ full length domain indices and subindices): Summary rating scales in seven domains were completed by using an interviewer and a "reader-rater" for 484 of the 590 respondents and by an interviewer and interview observer for the remaining 106. According to Lawton et al,: "In the sample of 484, interviewers and reader-raters agreed with either a 0 or 1 point discrepancy in 95% of all instances: intraclass correlations ranged from .88 (Activities of Daily Living) to a low .58 (Social 22 Interaction). Reliability (Alpha) of the final MAI scales were all .61 or above except for health behavior (.39) and personal security (.57). Retest reliability, done at a 3 week interval on 22 cases, was acceptable, with the exception of the physical self-maintenance subindex, where the variability was very low, the majority receiving a perfect score." (p. 95) mm was determined by doing summary ratings, multiple correlations, and by constructing a "dummy variable" representing independent versus dependent groups. The only domain that appeared somewhat problematic was the social domain. Lawton et al conclude that the reliabilities and validities of the MAI indices and subindices were affirmed by several different approaches. W: Because analysis has been done separately for each domain, portions of the MAI need not be used if desired. The authors maintain that physical health, cognition, and activities of daily living are the strong domains. They acknowledge that the social interaction scale is somewhat deficient psychometrically and suggest using the friends and family subindices separately. This researcher has separated the support people into specific categories (e.g. brothers, sisters, sons, daughters, etc). In addition, two questions were added pertaining to the presence and helpfulness of confidants. 2) W (BDI) (Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J., 1960). This inventory has been studied for reliability and validity in the elderly (Gallagher, Nies, 6: Thompson, 1982; Gallagher, Breckenridge, Steinmetz, 8: Thompson, 1983). The BDI is a 21 item, 4-point discrimination scale that rates the intensity of depression symptoms. Psychometric properties of the scale are 23 reviewed in Beck (1967). Results of studies with the elderly show that the BDI has respectable internal consistency and stability for use in research with this population. For example, congruence between conventional cut off scores on the BDI and selected diagnostic classifications of the Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) (Spitzer, Endicott, Gr Robins, 1978) for detection of Major and Minor Depressive Disorders in 102 elderly outpatients was high. Only 16.7% were misclassified by customary BDI cutoff scores (Gallagher et al, 1983). These results suggest that the BDI can also be used as a screening instrument for identification of clinically depressed elders. 3) W (351) (Derogatis 6: Spencer, 1983). This instrument is a 53 item abbreviated version of the SCL-90 (Derogatis, 1977). According to Derogatis and Spencer, correlations between the two range from .92 to .99. The 851 measures psychopathology along nine dimensions, including depression. Norms are available for the elderly grouped by gender (Hale, Cochran, & Hedgepeth, 1984). Internal consistency ranges from alpha = .77 on the Interpersonal Sensitivity Scale to .91 on the Depression Scale. One difference noted between the elderly and younger adults is that the elderly tend to report higher levels of distress on most symptom dimensions (Hale et al, 1984). Hale et al suggest, therefore, that age-relevant norms be used when the B81 is employed with the elderly population. 4) WW (SDAT) (Storandt, Botwinick, Danzinger, Berg, Gr Hughes, 1984). The SDAT battery is a brief (10 minute) battery of four psychological tests used to assess actual memory functioning. The battery consists of Mental Control and Logical 24 Memory from the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1955), Trailmaking A, which is part of the Halstead—Reitan neuropsychological battery, and the Word Fluency test which requires people to list as many words as possible, beginning with a specific letter, in 60 seconds. Eight-four individuals, 38 men and 46 women, half of whom were diagnosed as having mild SDAT and half normal controls were recruited from local physicians or in response to widespread community announcement of the project. Those with mild SDAT ranged in age from 63 to 81 years (mean :1: SD, 71.5 :t 5.0 years). Years of education ranged from 8 to 21 (12.4 :t 4.1 years). Healthy matched controls ranged in age from 64-82 years (71.6 13.2 years). Their range of education was 8 to 20 (12.4 :1: 5.2 years). The sample was partitioned into two subsamples by selecting every other matched pair in the order enrolled in the study. An initial discriminant analysis was performed on one set of 21 pairs. The regression weights were then applied to the second set. The first discriminant analysis correctly classified all persons on the basis of the four variables described above. Applying the weights to the second set, all but two persons (one demented, one not) were classified correctly (98% correct). Standardized coefficients were used for the final analysis. Canonical scores greater than or equal to 0 were classified as demented; those with canonical scores less than 0 were classified as normal. RESULTS SUBJECT CHARACTERISTICS Demographic data were collected for the following categories: Age, gender, marital status, and education. In addition, information regarding health, memory, housing, and psychological well-being was also obtained. AM One hundred sixty-five participants ranging in age from 55 to 91 (Mean, Median, Mode = 71; SD. = 6.8) agreed to take part in the research. Although the majority of the participants (109) were female; nevertheless, there is still a representative sample of males (56). The percentage of females to males is 66% to 34%. Housing, One indication of the independence of these participants is that 75% lived in single family homes and 21 % resided in apartments. Only 4% were in housing specifically for the elderly. In addition, the majority lived with at least one other person, with only 39% percent living alone. MW While the majority of the sample was married, the percentage was not overwhelming (58%), with 31% widowed and the remaining 11 % either divorced or never married. As expected, most individuals were not in the work force; however, almost 25% were employed at least part-time. Edugag'gn. This is a relatively well-educated sample with 76% having attended college. Of these, about 80% obtained at least an Associate's degree and over 45% earned a Master's degree or higher. Health, In general, this sample of older adults viewed their overall health as good or excellent (83%) with only 3% rating themselves in poor 25 26 health. An overwhelming majority (95 %) left their neighborhood at least once per week and were able to drive a car. In addition, 80% felt their health was the same or better than 3 years ago with over 90% percent stating their health either had none or virtually no impact on their ability to engage in activities. In addition, when asked to rate their overall health compared to others of the same age, over 50% rated their health as better than most people. Within the past 12 months, 83% had not been in the hospital; over 70% had been sick in bed one day or less; and the average number of reported visits to the doctor in the past year was 5.5 (SD = 7.2) with the median 3 and the mode 2. The total number of illnesses in the past year ranged from 0 to 11 with the mean and median 2 (SD = 1.7) and the mode 1. Of the illnesses, arthritis was most frequently listed (54%), followed by hypertension (33%), and cataracts (20%). Eighty-four percent took at least one medication with the range 0 to 11, the mean and median 3 (SD = 2.1), and the mode 2. m Twenty-six percent indicated they experienced major memory problems in the past and 70% of these indicated memory problems were still present. Eleven percent admitted to difficulty in the past with knowing the time of day, day of the week, or month of the year, with over 60% of these continuing to have this problem. Actual memory functioning as measured by the SDAT battery suggested that 11 % of the sample needed further follow-up to determine if they were showing signs of early dementia; the rest of the participants scored within the normal range on this test (Scores ranged from -6.380 to +2096 with the mean -1.908 and 8.0. 1.625). 27 W Results on the two tests of psychological well-being indicated that this sample is relatively well-adjusted with few reporting significant depression or other mental health symptoms. For examwe, scores on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) ranged from 0 to 33 with the mean score 6.5 (SD = 5.3). Scores less than or equal to 10 are considered normal for this measure of depression. Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) scores for all subscales were lower than the norms for the elderly except for the Obsessive-Compulsive and Interpersonal Sensitivity scales which were slightly higher than those reported in Hale et al (1984). Overall scores on the BSI ranged from .00 to 1.62 with the Mean .38 (SD = 0.32). CONSTRUCTION OF SCALES As stated earlier, the purpose of this research is to look at the relationships among mm W and W W Life is measured by social support, memory complaints, and perception of health; thagigral Competence is measured by actual health and memory functioning; W is measured by reported mental health status. In order to form scales that would measure these constructs, factor analysis using Principle Axis Factoring (PAF) with a varimax rotation was performed on items from the Multi—level Assessment Instrument (MAI) pertaining to memory complaints, perception of health, social support, and actual health. In addition, items on the MAI that pertain to depression and other symptoms of mental health status along with scores on the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) were factor analyzed to form a scale measuring mental health status. 28 Based on the above analyses, four scales were developed: Momory m 1 ° ' al a1 Heal and Mental Hoalth. The following are the scales that form the constructs Pergeivfi Quality L_e.fLif Wmdwk 1 'al ll-Be' : PERCEIVED QUALITY OF LIFE W- Given that none of the Social Support questions loaded significantly on any one factor, it was decided to add the total number of relevant items from the MAI (38b through 38h) to establish this scale. This included items which listed types of people within the social network (daughters, sons, friends, etc), frequency of visits to and from these individuals, number of phone calls, physical distance of support people, number of confidants, and the ability of confidants to provide emotional support. (See Table 2 for the actual categories). Note: All the items in this table refer to individuals not living with the subject; hence, there is no category for "spouse." (Higher scores indicate more social support). WW5 scale was established by summing the raw scores of items 24, 24a, 25, and 25a from the MAI (factor loadings ranged from .69 to .77 ). The internal consistency of the scale was found to be more than satisfactory (Cronbach's alpha = .85, item-total correlations ranged from .67 to .72). (Higher scores indicate more memory complaints). W This scale was established by summing the standardized scores of items 14 and 15 from the MAI (factor loadings ranged from .44 to .46 ). The internal consistency of the scale was satisfactory (Cronbach's alpha = .59, correlation between these two items =.42). (Higher scores indicate more positive perception of health). 29 TABLE 2 SOCIAL SUPPORT VARIABLES V A LE E MEAN .D. MEDIAN M DE % Daughters 0-4 1.03 1.06 1 . 0 Sons 0—5 0.85 1.02 1 0 Grandchild 0-4 0.18 0.63 0 0 Sisters 0-3 0.41 0.73 0 0 Brothers 0-4 0.28 0.62 0 0 Female 0-6 1.65 1.61 1 0 Friends Male 0-6 0.75 1.23 0 0 Friends Total 0-8 6.20 1.98 7 8 Persons Number of 0-8 5.30 2.28 5 8 Confidants Phone 0-1152 346.8 255.5 290 230 Calls/ Year Visits per 0-3740 658.0 517.1 518 128 Year 39 9O 33 0.6 3 O BEHAVIORAL COMPETENCE Wealth, This scale was established by summing the standardized scores of items 17, 18, 19, 30b, and the total number of illnesses listed from items 2a through 22t of the MAI (factor loadings ranged from .32 to .61). The internal consistency of the scale was satisfactory (Cronbach's alpha = .62, item- total correlations ranged from .24 to .47). (Lower scores indicate better overall health). mm Memory Function was measured by the overall score on the SDAT battery described above. (Lower scores indicate better memory functioning). PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING mm This scale was created by summing the standardized scores of the BDI and the BSI (factor loadings = .78 and .96). The internal consistency of the scale was more than satisfactory (Cronbach's alpha = .85, correlation between these two subscales = .73). (Lower scores indicate greater Psychological Well-being). The hypotheses were tested using two forms of analyses: Bivariate Analysis which examines the relationship between individual variables, and Multiple Regression Analysis which examines a set of independent (Predictor) variables as they relate to one single dependent (Criterion) variable. BIVARIATE ANALYSIS RESULTS The reader will recall Table 1 which lists the expected direction of the relationship between the following variables (these results can also be found in Table 3): 31 mm It was hypothesized that age would be significantly correlated with Actual Health and Social Support. The correlation between Actual Health and age was non-significant. The only variable in the scale Actual Health that was significantly correlated with age was total number of illnesses in the past year (1 = .16; p < .02). Two support items were significantly correlated with age: Number of sons (3; = .13; p < .04), and number of female friends (1; =- .12; p < .02). W: In addition, Memory Function (1 = .20; p < .004) and Memory Complaints (3; = .15; p < .03) were significantly correlated with age . My: Older individuals listed more sons and female friends, and as age increased, memory functioning decreased and memory complaints increased, as did number of illnesses. However, other indicators of health (number of days in bed, etc) were not significantly correlated with age. MW It was hypothesized that Actual Health would be significantly correlated with the following variables: Age, Perception of Health, Memory Complaints, Social Support, and Psychological Well-Being. As noted above, age was not significantly related to Actual Health; however, the other hypotheses were all confirmed: Perception of Health (1; = - .49; p < .000); Memory Complaints (r = .31; p < .000); Psychological Well-Being (; a .43; p < .000). Social Support was significantly correlated with Actual Health in the predicted direction (except for number of grandchildren). The Social Support items that were significant are as follows: Number of grandchildren (1: = .16; p < .027); number of daughters (1: = -.15; p < .037); total persons (r = -.17; p < .020); phone calls (r = -.15; p < .034); number of confidants (r; = -.18; p < .013). All 32 other measures of Social Support were not significantly related to Actual Health. ' Wm In addition to the predicted variables, the following were also significantly correlated with Actual Health: Memory Function Q = .22; p < .003); number of years in school (r = -.15; p < .032); attendance at college (1; = -.15; p < .034). Summgy: Participants reporting better overall health were more likely to have attended college or have spent more time in school. They performed better on memory tests, had fewer memory complaints, fewer mental health problems, and viewed their health more positively. They also listed more people and more confidants in their social network. MW It was predicted that Actual Health, Memory Complaints, Psychological Well-being, and Social Support would be significantly correlated with Perception of Health. All of the hypotheses were confirmed: Actual Health (r = -.49; p < .000); Memory Complaints (r = -.30; p < .000); Psychological Well-being (r; = -.45; p < .000). Individuals' perception of health was correlated with the following social support variables: Number of children (r; = .17; p < .017); number of friends (1: = .13; p < .05); total number of persons (1 = .19; p < .008); number of confidants (r a: .18; p < .010); amount of emotional support (r = .17; p < .016). W: In addition to the predicted variables, Memory Function was also significantly correlated with Perception of Health (r = -.26; p < .000). m: Participants with more positive perception of health reported fewer memory problems and mental health concerns as well as greater actual health. They also performed better on memory function tests. 33 In addition, these individuals listed more children, friends, and total number of people to whom they felt close, in whom they could confide, and from whom they felt emotional support. WWW It was predicted that Perception of Health, Memory Function, and Psychological Well-being would all be significantly correlated with Memory Complaints. These hypotheses were confirmed: Perception of health (r = -.30; p < .000); Memory Function (r = .27; p < .000); and Psychological Well-being (r = .30; p < .000). W: Amount of emotional support (r = -.21; p < .022) and number of confidants (r = .18; p < .01) were also significantly correlated with Memory Complaints. 5mm: Complaints of memory were associated with an increase in reported mental health problems and a decrease in subjects' perception of their health as well as their reported actual health status. In addition, presence of emotional support and number of confidants were correlated with fewer memory complaints. ‘Wflmmmrh It was hypothesized that Memory Complaints and Psychological Well-being would be significantly correlated with Memory Function. Both of these predictions were confirmed: Memory Complaints (see above), and Psychological Well-being (l; = .22; p < .003). W: As noted above, Perception of Health and Actual Health were significantly correlated with Memory Function. Other variables include attendance at college (1: =- -.25; p < .001); years in college (r = -.19; p < .017); and overall years in school (r = -.19; p < .009). 34 In addition, the following social support variables were significantly correlated with Memory Function: Number of daughters (r = -.13; p < .052); number of brothers (1 = -.13; p < .051); total number of persons (g = -.14; p < .038); number of confidants (1; a -.26 p < .001); and amount of emotional support (r; = -.21 p < .004). 5311313311: As Memory Function scores increased, so did actual health and perception of health; whereas, memory complaints and mental health - problems decreased. In addition, attendance at college, number of years in college, overall years in school, as well as having people to confide in, feel close to, and receive emotional support from increased the likelihood of better memory function. WW2. It was hypothesized that Actual health, Perception of Health, Memory Complaints, and Memory Function would be significantly correlated with Psychological Well-being. As described above, all of these hypotheses were confirmed. The following social support variables were significantly correlated with Psychological Well-being: Number of daughters (1 = -.20; p < .006); number of sons (1 = -.15; p < .030); number of female siblings (r = -.13 p < .050); number of confidants (r; = -.28; p < .000); and amount of emotional support (1; = -.24; p < .001). Visits, phone calls, or physical distance from the social network were not significantly related to Psychological Well-being; however, number of years in school (1 = -.21; p < .004); and college attendance (r = -.24; p < .001) were significantly correlated with Psychological Well-being. 53mm: Participants in better overall health who had few if any memory complaints, performed well on memory tests and saw their health more positively, reported fewer mental health concerns. In addition, fewer 35 daughters, sons, sisters, and confidants, as well as less emotional support were all negatively correlated with Psychological Well-being. Whether or not there was frequent contact via phone calls or visits, or how far away these support people were did not affect perception of mental health; rather, the presence of supportive individuals and feeling emotional support were the significant factors. Finally, number of years in school and having attended college were positively correlated with Psychological Well-being. AGE AH MC MF PWB COLY DGHTR SON F.FR. M.FR. BRO SIS DS L agesasfl 36 CODE FOR TABLE 3 AGE OF PARTICIPANT ACTUAL HEALTH PERCEPTION OF HEALTH MEMORY COMPLAINTS MEMORY FUNCTION PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING TOTAL YEARS IN SCHOOL COLLEGE ATTENDANCE TOTAL YEARS IN COLLEGE NUMBER OF DAUGHTERS LISTED NUMBER OF SONS LISTED NUMBER OF CHILDREN LISTED NUMBER OF FEMALE FRIENDS LISTED NUMBER OF MALE FRIENDS LISTED NUMBER OF BROTHERS LISTED NUMBER OF SISTERS LISTED NUMBER OF GRANDCHILDREN LISTED TOTAL NUMBER OF PERSONS LISTED TOTAL NUMBER OF PHONE CALLS PER YEAR TOTAL NUMBER OF CONFIDANTS AMOUNT OF EMOTIONAL SUPPORT NUMBER OF MEDICATION S TAKEN PER DAY TOTAL NUMBER OF ILLNESSES IN PAST YEAR 37 TABLE 3 BIVARIATE ANALYSES RESULTS AGE AH PH MC; MF PWB AGE - AH N.S. - PH N.S. -.49"" - MC .15" .31"" -.30"" - MF .20“ .22“ -.26"" .27“ - PWB N.S. .43“ -.45"" .30“ .22“ - SCH N.S. -.15"" N.S. N.S. -.19"" -.21"" COL N.S. -.15" N.S. N.S. -.25"" -.24"" COLY N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. -.19"" N.S. DGHTR N.S. -.15" N.S. N.S. .13" -.20"" SON .13" N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. -.15"" CH N .S. N.S. .1 7" N .S. N.S. -.24"" F.FR. .12" N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. M.FR. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. BRO N.S. N.S. .13" N.S. N.S. N.S. SIS N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. -.I3" GC N.S. .16" N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. TP N.S. -.17" .19" N.S. .14"" -.27"" PC N.S. -.15" N.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. CF N.S. -.18"" .18"" -.I8"" -.26"" -.28"" ES N .S. N .S. .17" -.21" -.21"" -.24"" MEDS N.S. + -.34"" .18" N.S. .34“ TILL .16" + -.35"" .18" 23"" .33“ "P (.05 ""P<.01 + part of scale 38 GENDER DIFFERENCES In addition to the bivariate analyses reported above, cross tabulation of sex by marital status by confidant was performed to test the following hypothesis: When asked to name a confidant, (who may or may not live with the subject) females and males will differ in who they believe is someone they can talk to about their troubles or concerns: FEMALES: Friends '> Spouse > siblings > children MALES: Spouse > children > siblings > friends Results of the cross tabulation analysis showed that 89% of the males and 83% of the females listed a confidant. 92% of these males were married as compared to 41% of the females. The remaining 8% of males were widowed. More widowed women (44%) than married women named a confidant. In addition, of those women who named a confidant, 9% were divorced and the remaining 6% had never been married. Given that the original hypothesis included listing a spouse as confidant, the following gender difference analysis is between married males and females. The result is statistically significant at the <.000 level: MARRIED FEMALES: Spouse (59%) > Friend (30%) > Child (11 %) MARRIED MALES: Spouse (87%) > Friend (6%) > Child (2%) = Sibling (2%) = Other (2%) The original hypothesis was not confirmed. Both, genders were more apt to name their spouse as confidant; however, more married men (87 %) as compared to married women (59%) did so, and only 6% of married men as compared to 30% of married women listed a friend as their confidant. Given that 44% of those women who named a confidant were widowed, a post hoc analysis evaluated the response of widowed women: 39 WIDOWED WOMEN: Child (50%) > Friend (37%) > Other relative (7%) > Sibling (2%) = Other (2%) Thus, half the widowed women in this sample who named a confidant listed one of their children, and over one-third of them named a friend. MULTIPLE REGRESSION RESULTS In order to test the formal hypotheses, forward multiple regression was performed on the following predictor variables with Psychological Well-being as the criterion variable: Perception of Health and Actual Health; Memory Complaints and Memory Function; and Social Support variables. Once the significant predictors of Psychological Well-being were identified, the demographic variables (age, gender, marital status, education) were then regressed onto these "predictors" of Psychological Well-being in a separate analysis. These same demographic variables were also entered as predictors of Psychological Well-being. Once the significant demographic variables were identified, the original multiple regression analyses were run again using both the significant demographic variables and the original predictor variables (Memory Complaints, Memory Function, Actual Health, etc.) with Psychological Well-being as the criterion. (See Figure 1) Finally, the demographic variables and Social Support individuals (e. g. daughters, sons, friends) were entered as predictors of amount of emotional support. The same model of regression analysis described above was performed with social support individuals (e.g. sons, daughters) as the predictors and the amount of emotional support as the criterion. (See Figure 1) 4o DEMOGRAPHICS Age Gender Education Marital Status VARIOUS PREDICTORS fl PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING (e.g. Memory Complaints, , or Actual Health, EMOTIONAL SUPPORT Social Support) M Figure 1 MULTIPLE REGRESSION MODEL 41 See Table 4 for the statistical data on the following results. HYPOTI-IESIS #1: Both Perception of Health and Actual Health will have an effect on Psychological Well-being; that is, positive perception of health as well as better overall health will be positively correlated with Psychological Well-being. Results of multiple regression analysis confirm the above hypothesis. Both of these variables have a unique effect on Psychological Well-being. That is, if Actual Health (or Perception of Health) is held constant, Perception of Health (or Actual Health) continues to be a significant predictor of Psychological Well-being whiCh suggests that Perception of Health is measuring something that Actual Health is not and vice versa. This will be discussed below. In addition, the combination of Actual Health and Perception of Health is a better predictor of Psychological Well-being than either of these variables alone; however given that Perception of Health entered the multiple regression equation first, it is likely that this variable has a greater effect on Psychological Well-being than Actual Health. Results of entering the demographic variables in the multiple regression analysis indicate that attendance at college (which is a significant predictor of Psychological Well-being) has an effect over and above that of Actual Health and Perception of Health. Thus, those who attended college, had better overall health and had a more positive perception of their health were more likely to score better on measures of psychological well-being. No demographic variables directly predicted either Perception of Health or Actual Health. HYPOTHESIS #2: Memory complaints will have a stronger relationship to Psychological Well-being than will Memory Function; 42 although there will be a significant correlation between Memory Function and Psychological Well-being. This hypothesis was confirmed. If Memory Function is held constant, Memory Complaints continues to have a significant effect on Psychological Well-being; however, if Memory Complaints is held constant, Memory Function ceases to be significantly related to Psychological Well-being; thus, given that Memory Complaints and Memory Function are significantly correlated with each other, Memory Function appears to have an indirect effect on Psychological Well-being through its direct effect on Memory Complaints. Attendance at college is the only demographic variable that continues to be a significant predictor of Psychological Well-being when Memory Complaints is entered into the multiple regression equation; that is, those who attended college and have fewer memory complaints are more likely to have positive psychological well-being. No demographic variables significantly affected either Memory Complaints or Memory Function when they were analyzed via multiple regression. In addition, Memory Function which appears to directly affect Psychological Well-being when analyzed by simple Pearson Product Moment Correlation, does not directly relate to Psychological Well-being; rather, in the Multiple Regression Analysis, it appears to affect one's perception of memory (Memory Complaints) and this perception in turn affects Psychological Well-being. HYPOTHESIS #3 The presence and perceived helpfulness of a confidant will act as a buffer or moderator between Perception of Health and Psychological Well-being. 43 The above hypothesis was confirmed for presence of confidants but not for perceived helpfulness of confidants; however, attendance at college had a stronger effect on Psychological Well-being than did presence of confidants. That is, those who attended college had a more positive perception of their health and if they also had confidants, their overall Psychological Well-being was significantly greater than those who had not attended college or had fewer confidants. An added factor appears to be gender in that females reported significantly more confidants in their social network than did males; therefore gender through its effect on number of confidants appears to have an indirect effect on Psychological Well-being. HYPOTHESIS #4: Friends and siblings will provide more emotional support than children, especially for females. This hypothesis was not confirmed. Results of the multiple regression analysis reveal that, of individuals who subjects felt close to and who did not live with them, the following is the order of importance in terms of emotional support: Daughters > Female friends > Sons > Brothers > Sisters > Male friends In addition, females were more apt to name daughters and female friends; whereas, males were more apt to name male friends. Also, attendance at college and marital status may have an indirect effect on emotional support from daughters in that these variables were predictors of number of daughters in the network. Attendance at college was also a predictor of emotional support from daughters and female friends. Given the above results, the original hypothesis was not confirmed in that daughters are most helpful followed by female friends and then sons. However, females in general appear to be more helpful than males. In 44 addition, there appears to be two distinct groups with the most helpful group being daughters, female friends, and sons (in that order) and the less helpful group being brothers, sisters, and male friends (in that order). The least helpful of all are male friends which were listed more by males; whereas, the two most helpful (daughters and female friends) were listed more by females. (See Table 5) HYPOTHESIS # 5: Social support variables and their effect on psychological well-being will be in the following order of importance: Emotional support > number of confidants > number of phone calls per year > number of visits to and from individuals in the social network per year > total number of persons in the network > physical distance from individuals in the network Results of the multiple regression analysis indicate that, while both emotional support and number of confidants were significant predictors of Psychological Well-being, none of the other variables have any significant effect. In addition, when the demographic variables are analyzed for their effect, attendance at college appears to be significantly related to number of confidants; therefore, it appears that, for this sample, it doesn't matter how often there is contact with supportive people in the network or how close one lives to these people; rather, the number of confidants and perceived helpfulness of them are the significant correlates of Psychological Well-being and if individuals have attended college, they are more likely to have such individuals in their network (Table 4). 45 TABLE 4 MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSES WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING AS THE DEPENDENT VARIABLE PREDICTORS BETA Fér'céfiti'o'n-Sf'fiéfit'h""""""""""'""?0’.§§? """"" Actual Health 0.26"" College Attendance -0.16" R2 0.30 F(3, 145) 21.04"" Fér'cé'fititit'ai-fiéfith""""""""""""-TdI§7-*7 """""" College Attendance -0.19"" Number of Confidants -0.16"" Emotional Support -0.15(n.s.) R2 0.25 F(3, 153) 17.05"" Memo-ry-Cdn-rpliihts """"""""""""" (its: “““““ Memory Function 0.09(n.s.) College Attendance 024"" R2 0.14 F(2,155) 12.63"" 4 6 TABLE 4 (cont'd) PREDICTORS BETA ETmBTiBHa'l'§tI§pB'§t-""""'""""""""?0'.§4?‘ -------- Number of Confidants n.s Phone Calls per Year n.s Visits per Year n.s Number of Persons in n.s Network Physical distance from n.s Network R2 0.06 F(1, 155) 9.80"" CBEEg-e-At-tend—ahze----—---—------__-—_—-—-_:0_.24-"; """"""" Number of Confidants -0.21"" Emotional Support n.s Phone Calls per Year n.s Visits per Year n.s Number of Persons in n.s Network Physical distance from n.s Network R2 0.12 F(2, 153) 10.47"" :.---<-751- “““““““““““““““““““““““““ 47 TABLE 5 MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSIS WITH EMOTIONAL SUPPORT AS THE DEPENDENT VARIABLE PREDICTORS BETA bitigitér's ---------------------------- 0 '55:? """"""" Female Friends 0.46"" Sons ' 0.38"" Brothers 0.24"" Sisters 0.17"" Male Friends 0.15" R2 0.34 F(6,153) 13.12"" rat-5231' """""""""""""""""""""""" DISCUSSION The purpose of this research was to examine the correlates of psychological well-being in the able elderly; that is, those individuals over age 55 who were living independently in the community. The frame of reference was Lawton's (1986) conceptualization of "The Good Life" for this population (see Introduction). mm W as measured by Perception of Health, Memory Complaints, and Social Support; havi r m n as measured by Actual Health and Memory Function; and MEI—9832131111: mg as measured by the amount of mental health concerns and symptoms were the three aspects of this "Good Life" analyzed by this researcher. It should be noted that Lawton does not differentiate between Memory Function and Memory Complaints in his model and subsumes both of these under W. Given that Memory Complaints is a subjective evaluation of cognitive functioning, while Memory Function is an objective measure, this researcher has opted to separate these two constructs. Lawton's conceptualization of Memory Function as a measure of Bohavioral Qompotongo appears warranted; however, Memory Complaints can be argued to be more of a subjective evaluation (a perception) of Major-31 W and therefore, more a measure of m W. In addition, Lawton includes both Perception of Health and Actual Health as measures of W. As in the case of memory discussed above, it is reasonable to assume that perception of health is more of a cognitive gyoluoo'oo of actual health and thus a measure of Porooivoo W rather than one of ha ' ral m t n e. Therefore, the decision was made to separate these two constructs as well. 48 49 Finally, even though Lawton conceptualizes number of relatives, friends, etc as part of the oneoo'yo Enyironment, given that subjects in this research were asked to name those individuals they "felt close to", these variables also appear to be a better measure of W and are thus considered part of this aspect of the "Good Life." The ijootiyg Eovironmont portion of Lawton's model was not examined in the present research. SUBJECT CHARACTERISTICS Specific characteristics of the participants who agreed to take part in the research project can be found in the Results Section. In general, there is little doubt that the men and women who volunteered were a representative sample of able elderly, those living independently in the community. Only 4% of them lived in housing specifically for the elderly and three—fourths lived in private homes. Virtually all (95%) left their neighborhood at least once per week and were able to drive a car. The majority of participants were either married or widowed, with only 11 % divorced or never married. This may represent a cohort effect as remaining single or being divorced was not as acceptable when these individuals were young adults. As might be expected, the majority of participants were retired, although almost 25% continued to work at least part-time. The educational background of these individuals was obviously of an advanced level, with over 75% having attended college. In addition, of those who attended college, over 45% obtained a Master's degree or higher. It seems likely that this result is due to the the majority of volunteers coming from the retired staff or faculty of MSU (as well as their spouses). 50 A criticism of this research, therefore, might be that these participants are not representative of the overall population of elderly, and as a result, the findings are limited in generalizability. While this may be a valid point, one can look at this aspect of the subject population from another perspective. Schaie (1988) has argued that, in fact, there is a "great diversity" in today's elderly. In fact, he cautions researchers not to assume that all elderly over age 60 are alike merely because they are classified as such. If his comments bear consideration, then examining different groups of elderly demonstrates responsible research, and one subset of the elderly that investigators need to study is the highly educated individuals who are both physically and psychologically healthy and can and should continue to be positive contributors to society. That this is a group of able elderly is best seen in the data regarding health, memory and psychological well-being. Only 3% rated themselves in poor health and over 80% saw their health as good or excellent. Only 20% thought their health was worse than three years ago and virtually all (90%) stated their health had little or no impact on their ability to engage in activities. Most of them had not even been sick in bed one day in the past 12 months, and the most common illnesses listed were typical consequences of the aging process (hypertension, arthritis, cataracts), rather than life threatening conditions such as cancer or strokes. Concerns with memory do not appear to be a major issue with this sample of elderly. For example, only about 25% felt they had major memory problems in the past, with about 70% continuing to have this problem. In addition, only 11% admitted to problems with the time of day, day of week, or month of year, with only 60% of these individuals feeling this was a problem at the time of the interview. It may be, however, that had there been more 51 questions about memory or that the the word "major" had not been chosen, more individuals might have expressed concerns about this aspect of their functioning. One possibility would be to use a scale like the one proposed by Crook and Larrabee (1990) which has a wide range of memory items, a large normative base, and an "extremely stable factor structure" (p.53). The test of actual memory function revealed that this sample of elderly did not have severe memory problems (only 1 1 % needed further follow-up to screen for early signs of dementia); therefore, even with more detailed memory complaint questions, it is likely that the percentage of subjects admitting to problems would remain small. On the other hand, it should be recognized that there is no normative data which relates memory complaints in the highly educated able elderly to performance on memory tests. Until data of that nature is collected, the relationship between memory complaints and actual performance is an open question. Despite the suggestion by professionals that depression may be a major concern for the elderly (Blazer, Hughes, & George, 1987); few of the elderly who took part in this project reported significant depression or other mental health problems. However, even though moderate to severe psychological symptoms were relatively absent in this sample of elderly; nevertheless, results discussed below indicate that even relatively mild symptoms are affected by such things as memory complaints and social support. There are several possible explanations for the virtual absence of depression in these participants: Newman (1989) suggests that elderly people who participate in most community based research are likely to be "disproportionately represented by W" (p.162). Given that this was the very sub-population of elderly the current study sought to investigate, the 52 absence of major depressive symptoms in the sample can be looked on as a positive sign that these elderly were indeed "optimal agers." Another "explanation" can be found in the recent longitudinal study by Aldwin, Spiro, Levenson, and Bosse (1989) who examined the question of whether mental health changes with age. They found that psychological symptoms were "essentially stable and increased at a rate of one new symptom every 28 years" (p.303). In addition, they found no linear relationship between age and psychological symptoms; although they did report non-linear changes in the number of symptoms with symptom reporting increasing in young adulthood, remaining stable in mid-life and then increasing slightly in older age. It is possible, therefore, that this sample of able elderly were also relatively well-adjusted throughout their lives. In support of this explanation is the fact that there may be an underestimation of depression in the general population because research has relied on traditional psychiatric diagnosis (Blazer, Hughes, 8: George, 1987) rather than standard screening scales to estimate depression (Newman, 1989). Given that the present study did in fact utilize two standard screening scales (The Brief Symptom Inventory and the Beck Depression Inventory) and failed to find significant psychological disturbance, it is likely that this sample is biased towards the healthy individual, which as stated above, was the intent of this research. Gatz and Hurwicz (1990) also report no significant age difference in level of depression and state: "...although conclusions must be tempered by their not being based on longitudinal data, there is not compelling confirmation of the common belief that depression inexorably increases decade by decade" (p.289). Gatz and Hurwicz also report that there was no relationship between their "Lack of Well-being" subscale and other 53 depression sub-scales in the oldest age group. They suggest that "expressions of lack of well-being may represent a construct different from depression" and that "older people may not be unduly depressed but rather coming to terms with some of the realistic constraints of their lives" (p.289). Finally, Lee and Ishii-Kuntz (1988) in their research on social interaction, loneliness, and emotional well-being in the elderly report that those with more education have a "higher morale" than others. Given the educational data reported above, this may also be a significant reason for the virtual lack of psychological dysfunction in the present sample. What appears likely, therefore, is that the present research has a disproportionate sample of individuals who are not only "optimal agers" but also have been psychologically healthy throughout their lives. While they may not be experiencing moderate or severe depression, they may be experiencing some discomfort over the inevitable changes that aging brings about. Further research is needed to examine this hypothesis. BIVARIATE RESULTS Even though the bivariate results reported above were not part of the formal hypotheses examined in this research, comments about these findings appear warranted. mom: Given Schaie's (1988) caution to avoid classifying all individuals in aging research as "over 60", it is, at first glance, somewhat surprising that age was not a significant demographic variable in this study. Indeed, age did not enter at all into any of the Multiple Regression equations that will be discussed below. Age only accounted for 3% of the variance in 54 total number of illnesses, 4% of the variance in Memory Function, and less than 2% of the variance in Memory Complaints. In addition, only about 1 % of the variance in number of sons and female friends could be attributed to age. The most likely explanation for the lack of differentiation by age in this population is that the sample was truncated; that is the mean age was 71 (SD. 6.8). This means that almost 90% of the sample could be classified as "Young-Old". Therefore, the logical explanation for a lack of age effect is that this is a homogeneous sample of elderly and the actual age differences among them have virtually no impact on the variables in the study due to the lack of variance in age. WM: Not surprisingly, Perception of Health had the strongest correlation with Actual Health and accounted for almost 25% of the variance. Thus, as would be expected, the healthier people are, the more positively they view their health. It also appears that Actual Health and Psychological Well-being are significantly related to one another which is consistent with prior research (e.g. Larue, Dessonville, & Jarvik, 1985; Blazer & Williams, 1980). In fact, some researchers found that poor physical health had the strongest association with depression (Murrell, I-Iimmelfarb & Wright, 1983). More will be said about this relationship when the regression analyses are discussed below. Given that it is not unusual for some illnesses to affect memory performance (e. g. diabetes, circulation problems), the finding that the memory scales and Actual Health were significantly related to each other is not surprising. 55 Although educational background was not hypothesized to be significantly correlated with Actual Health, the finding that more years in school and having attended college were related to better health makes sense if we can assume that these events are likely also correlated with higher socio- economic status. Unfortunately, data was not collected specifically about socio-economic status, but it is reasonable to expect that a sample of adults that came primarily from retired staff and faculty of a major university is largely from at least the middle if not upper-middle class. Finally, the expectation that more social support would be positively correlated with better health was confirmed; however, it apparently does not matter if face-to face contact takes place in that only number of phone calls was associated with better health. In addition, daughters, total number of people, and confidants were significantly (albeit weakly) associated with better health. Interestingly, number of grandchildren was negatively correlated with Actual Health. There does not appear to be an easy answer for this finding, unless perhaps younger people have more time and energy to spend with their sick elderly relatives. On the face of it, younger off-spring are initially more mobile before they have children. It might be helpful to examine the relationship of the elderly and their grandchildren in future research on social networks. MW: Perception of Health and its relationship to Actual Health was already discussed above. It was also expected that Perception of Health and Psychological Well-being would be significantly correlated with one another. This was confirmed by the bivariate analysis and is consistent with previous research which found that the meaning of health to individuals rather than actual health status may be what actually affects 56 psychological well-being (Murphy, 1983; Stroller, 1984). This hypothesis was examined in the multiple regression analysis to be discussed below. Of course, the assumption being made here is that the direction of the correlation is known, when in fact, it may be that depression affects the elderly’s perception of health rather than vice-versa. The finding that memory complaints and memory function are significantly related to perception of health in this sample of able elderly is likely due to the fact that cognitive deficits may signal poor health and this in turn would affect one's perception of health in general. The finding that many of the social support variables were significantly correlated with Perception of Health (e.g. children, friends, presence of confidants and emotional support) is consistent with research by Krause (1987). However, Krause differentiated between quantity and quality of support and found that quality rather than quantity was the significant factor. The present research mainly looks at quality of support in that subjects were told to list only those individuals in their network to whom they felt close. However, given that as the number of individuals who were listed increased, the more times participants could respond to the questions about confidants and amount of emotional Support, it appears that quano'ty of emotional support must also be taken into account in these findings. Therefore, it may not be just quality of support but also the quantity of this type of support that affects such factors as Perception of Health. W: In addition to the correlations discussed above, Memory Complaints were also significantly related to Psychological Well-being, Memory Function, amount of emotional support and number of confidants. 5 7 The relationship between Memory Complaints and Psychological Well-being found in the present study is consistent with previous research (Zarit, 1982; Williams, Little, Scates, 8: Blockman, 1987). In addition, although correlations between Memory Function and Memory Complaints have been reported (see Zarit), the overall conclusion is that Memory Complaints are more correlated with Psychological Well-being than with actual function. Though again we caution that there have been few normative studies on memory among the highly educated, well-functioning elderly. Given that this sample of elderly is highly educated and many of them may have made a living by the use of their cognitive abilities, it may be that even small changes in memory function are noticeable and therefore results in complaints of memory deterioration which could be real memory decrements. Post hoc analysis of soda] support and Memory Complaints reveals an interesting relationship. Those who complained of memory difficulties listed fewer confidants and less emotional support. Further research looking at the relationship between memory complaints and sodal networks is needed before any reliable explanation of this finding can be postulated. It may be, however, that individuals who are experiendng concerns about their competence, be it in the area of cognitive abilities or physical health, may be more aloof from their network or perceive others as non-supportive of their fears and concerns. Or, they may not confide their fears in others‘even if they feel "close" to them. Again, the memory complaints may be tool in the sense of actual memory impairments not found by gross testing of memory functions. W: In addition to the correlations discussed above, Memory Function was also found to be significantly related to 58 Psychological Well-being, overall years in school, attendance at college and number of years in college. In addition, amount of emotional support, number of confidants, daughters, brothers, and total number of persons subjects felt close to were significantly related to Memory Function. There are two possible explanations that may account for the correlation between Memory Function and Psychological Well=being. First of all, given that Memory Complaints and Memory Function were significantly correlated with one another, Memory Function may be affecting Psychological Well-being through its effect on Memory Complaints (this will be discussed below with the Multiple Regression Analyses). Secondly, the explanation given above for the relationship of memory complaints to memory function in this sample may also apply to memory function and its significant correlation to Psychological Well-being; that is, if even a small loss in memory function occurs in someone who is relatively well-educated, this loss could result in increased psychological symptoms as well. The fact that college attendance, years in college, and overall years in school also increase Psychological Well-being may be another indicator of the importance of cognitive functioning in this sample of well-educated elderly. It is interesting that Memory Function was also significantly related to such sodal support variables as number of daughters, number of confidants, and emotional support. If these people are functioning well cognitively, they may be reaching out to people more and thus receiving more support from others. W The relationship between Psychological Well-being, Actual Health, Perception of Health, Memory 59 Complaints, Memory Function and education has already been discussed above. The fact that number of confidants and amount of emotional support are related to positive mental health is consistent with prior research on the soda] network of the elderly (Krause, 1987; Esser 8r Vitaliano, 1988; Murphy, 1982). As discussed above, it appears that quality of support is the major factor that relates to Psychological Well-being. The fact that children and female siblings were the specific types of relatives is consistent with other findings (e.g. Ward, 1985; Cidrelli, 1989). Contrary to what might be expected, however, face-to-face or other types of contact were not significantly related to Psychological Well-being; thus, it doesn't seem to matter if the elderly visit or talk with their confidants, merely having them available per se appears to be the significant factor. Thus, Mm one has support and someone to talk with about troubles or concerns may be more important than actually sharing with that person. GENDER DIFFERENCES It was hypothesized that there would be a significant difference between males and females regarding whom they would name in answer to the question: "Is there someone you can talk to about your troubles or concerns (i.e. a confidant)." Contrary to what was expected, m males and females were more apt to name their spouse, although the percentage of males (87%) compared to females (59%) was greater, with almost all married men naming their wives, and only slightly over half of the women naming their husbands. It was thought that women would name a friend more often than any other individual, and although this was not confirmed, friends were the second most frequently named individual (30%) among married women. 60 In addition, women were more likely to name a friend than were men. Finally, only 11% of females and 2% of males named one of their children. The results for males, therefore, is consistent with other research (e. g. Vaux, 1985) in that males most often named their wives. For women, however, even though friends were often named, nevertheless, husbands were still in the majority. One possible explanation for this finding is sampling bias. Many of the volunteers came with their spouse; that is, both partidpated in the research, and even though the interviews were conducted separately and neither was obligated to tell the other how they answered any questions, there may have been some "pressure" to name a husband or wife. On the other hand, given that these elderly did come together, it is also possible that the couples were closer than the average married elderly and honestly felt their spouse was their closest confidant. In addition, perhaps educational background or sodo-economic status affects this variable. More research would be needed to better tease out the answer to this question. Discussion of the Multiple Regression analyses below will include further elaboration on possible gender differences in sodal support. Given that 44% of women who named a confidant were widows, these results are also included. Again, friends were not the source most often listed; rather, 50% of widows listed one of their children and 37% listed a friend. This is consistent with results found in other research of women in general (Depner & Ingersoll-Dayton, 1988), but not similar to a report by Arling (1976) who found that interaction with children had virtually no impact on the morale of widows. However, Arling did not spedfically look at quality of support, but rather frequency of interaction. Thus, the crucial variable regarding support may very well be quality rather than quantity. 6 1 MULTIPLE REGRESSION RESULTS _'=e "E010 l‘:--l £11-: rm-.. ._ OIO'HaI'A‘3‘ The results of the Multiple Regression analysis confirm the hypothesis that both Actual Health and Perception of Health significantly relate to Psychological Well-Being. If we examine the results more closely, we can state with a fair amount of confidence that, of the two, perception of health is the better predictor, but that both together account for the largest percentage of variance in Psychological Well-Being. These results are consistent with previous studies which found a significant relationship between perception of health and psychological well- being (Gatz & Hurwicz, 1990; Stroller, 1984), and others which reported a significant relationship between actual health and psychological well-being (LaRue et al., 1985; Murrell, I-Iimmelfarb 8: Wright, 1983). Although researchers may find that perception of health is invariably more salient to elderly people than actual evidence of good versus poor health; nevertheless, we cannot eliminate the effect of real health problems anymore than we can ever totally separate nature from nurture in attempting to explain personality, behavior, etc That is, one does not exist without the other. Thus, two elderly people, one in good health, the other in fair health may conceivably 9.239.112 their health in similar manners even though one person's health is objectively worse than the other's. This perception in turn may result in similar reports of mental health status. Finally, the fact that college attendance was the only demographic variable that entered into the regression equation further emphasizes the possible significance of the role higher education plays in the lives of individuals, the elderly in particular. It will be interesting to see how future 62 generations of elderly are affected by having attended college, espedally since the opportunity to do so is greater now than it was when the present sample of elderly were young adults. It may be that it will actually cease to be a predictor of well-being just because it will no longer be a somewhat unusual experience for people. P ‘s-sge gm“; 0 s. 1; secs;- sss ano,’ elm ushering: Results of the regression analysis confirm the original hypothesis that H the presence of confidants can add to the effects of Perception of Health on Psychological Well-being in the elderly. Whether these "confidants" are also viewed as "helpful" apparently does not add anything to this effect. Thus, it seems that if the elderly have a positive perception of their health and also have people they can talk to about their troubles or concerns, they are more apt to have greater psychological well-being. This is consistent with other research (e. g. Krause, 1987). In addition, if individuals have a somewhat less than positive perception of their health but also have confidants, they are more likely to be less psychologically disturbed by a somewhat negative perception of their health. That is, number of confidants "buffers" the effect of perception of health on psychological well-being. Further regression analysis using demographic variables to predict number of confidants revealed that females were more likely to name such individuals. This may suggest that a gender difference exists in the effect of perception of health on psychological well-being; that is, females may be less affected by a negative perception of health because they have more people to talk to about problems and concerns than men. However, given that there 6 3 was no significant gender interaction with psychological well-being in this sample, further research would be needed to test this hypothesis. One other explanation that comes to mind is that males may not need as many "confidants" in their overall network to feel psychologically healthy. Finally, an interesting result of entering the demographic variables in the original multiple regression equation is that college attendance actually was a better predictor of psychological well-being than was number of confidants. The difference was rather small (see Table 4), but nevertheless, this finding continues to raise an interesting question regarding the role of higher education in the lives of the able elderly. It appears that those who attended college were less likely to be psychologically distressed by the perception that their health was less than perfect. Or to put this another way, consistent with the findings of Lee 8: Ishii-Kuntz (1988), elderly with more education and better health appear to lead psychologically healthier lives than those with less education and poorer health. trains __!'1!°l trans 0910---Ib 1-10. ’ - 0 0°4C- A '--i‘ The hypothesis that Memory Complaints would have a stronger relationship to Psychological Well-Being than Memory Function was confirmed. In fact, if both of these are entered into a Multiple Regression equation, when Memory Complaints is held constant, Memory Function ceases to significantly affect Psychological Well-Being. This of course suggests that it is the elderly individual's subjective porceoo' on of memory function, not memory function per se that has an appredable effect on mental health. This is consistent with previous studies reported above. That Memory Function significantly affects Memory Complaints when simple Bivariate Correlation analysis is performed, however, suggests that 6 4 Memory Function does affect Psychological Well-being but that it is an indirect effect through its relationship to complaints of memory. As mentioned earlier, attendance at college was the only demographic variable that continued to be a significant predictor of Psychological Well- being when Memory Complaints was entered in the regression analysis. No demographic variables directly predicted Memory Complaints using regression or bivariate analysis. Interestingly, college attendance appears to buffer the effects of Memory Complaints on Psychological Well-being. Thus, the role that education plays continues to be unclear, although the trend seems to be that higher education increases the elderly's chances of being mentally healthy and happy. Further research is obviously warranted. W The hypofltesis that friends and siblings would provide more emotional support than children was not supported by this research. This sample of elderly named children, female friends, sons, brothers, sisters, and male friends (in that order). Furthermore, women were more apt to name daughters and men were more apt to name male friends. In addition, college attendance predicted number of daughters and amount of emotional support from daughters and female friends. The fact that daughters were the most frequently named providers of emotional support demonstrates that they continue to be important in the lives of their elderly parents. This is apparently not merely due to a sense of obligation. Elderly parents in the present sample see their daughters (and sons) as people they can "talk to about their troubles or concerns." The finding that females were more apt to name daughters and female friends is consistent with other studies (Antonucd 8r Akiyama, 1987). Thus, friends 65 assume an important role in the soda] network of the elderly, however, at least for the present population, they are not as important as daughters, even though female friends are somewhat more frequently listed than sons. When the regression results are looked at in more detail, two distinct groups of support people emerge, with daughters, female friends, and sons accounting for more emotional support than siblings and male friends. Siblings have been found in other research to be important individuals in the soda] network of the elderly (e. g. Cidrelli, 1990). One possible explanation for the difference found in the present study is that uner Cidrelli's research which only investigated the role of siblings, other individuals in the network were also examined. It may be that siblings are important, but that children and female friends assume a more important role in the lives of the able elderly. Another possibility is that siblings are "close" to each other, but the satisfaction with amount of emotional support is less than that with children or female friends. More research is needed to answer these questions. F‘mally, given that men were more apt to name males friends than children, siblings, or female friends, and that male friends had the weakest assodation with amount of emotional support, it is likely that elderly men have a smaller base of confidants than elderly women. Thinking back to the 87% of married males who listed a spouse as their one "confidant" as compared to 59% of married women, and that this research also points to a gender interaction with number of confidants (women having more than men), it appears that, if elderly individuals are told to name people they feel close to othir; than their spouse, men list fewer people, and the ones they do list tend to be less emotionally supportive. A somewhat surprising finding was the lack of relationship between actual contact with close family and friends and psychological well-being. As noted above, frequency of contact, espedally telephone calls have been shown to be significantly related to fewer mental health problems and greater life satisfaction in the elderly (Revicki 6: Mitchell, 1986; DeWit, Wister, & Burch, 1988). Results of the regression analysis, however, suggest that the only significant support variables are number of confidants and amount of emotional support. Even in the Bivariate results discussed above, there were no significant correlations between any other support measures and Psychological Well-being Given these results, it was dedded to see if contact with people in the support network possibly had an indirect effect on Psychological Well-being through its effect on confidants and amount of emotional support. Results of bivariate correlational analysis suggest that this is, in fact, the case. Number of phone calls was positively correlated with both number of confidants (; = .38; p < .000) and amount of emotional support (1; = .38; p < .000); total number of visits was positively correlated with number of confidants (r = .20; p < .005) and amount of emotional support (r; = .22; p < .003). A plausible explanation, ' therefore, is that amount of quality support directly affects psychological well- being in the able elderly, but that this is measured or evaluated by individuals through the amount of time spent either talking on the phone or visiting people they feel close to. In addition, the effect of college attendance on number of confidants may mean that those with higher education will find their sodal network filled with more people they can talk to about their troubles or concerns which in turn suggests that they will experience greater psychological well-being. 67 CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS How do the above findings contribute to our understanding of what constitutes "the Good Life" for the able elderly? If we re—examine Lawton's (1986) model using the results of the present research as predictors of this "Good Life" one fact stands out above the rest: W as measured by quality of support, perception of health, and memory complaints relates strongly to W. B havi ral m as measured by actual health and memory function has a direct effect on well- being through actual health status and an indirect effect through memory function, but nevertheless, a reasonable conclusion is that it is the moaning such conditions as deteriorating health and memory have for the elderly that affects their happiness and satisfaction with their lives. The aging process naturally brings about changes in health and cognitive functioning. For the able elderly, however, perceiving these changes as having little or no effect on their ability to engage in activities, coming from a better than average educational background, and feeling they can talk to people in their sodal networks about their troubles or concerns all contribute significantly to less depression and greater life satisfaction. The above conclusion assumes even greater importance when coupled with the fact that the present sample of elderly did not have major physical health, mental health, or memory problems. That is, even when health and/ or memory may not be significantly impaired, believing they are, or worrying about it results in more psychological distress for the able elderly. The role of soda] support must be acknowledged as vital to the well- being of this population. Having a confidant and feeling these confidants are good listeners not only contributes to psychological well-being, but also 6 8 buffers the effect that perception of health can have on such symptoms as depression. The findings of this research have contributed to our understanding of how soda] networks help the able elderly. Contrary to expectation, both married men and married women were more likely to list their husbands than other relatives or friends; however, when asked to name someone that was not living with them, daughters and female friends were more often dted by females and male friends were more often dted by males. Given that daughters and female friends have the largest correlation with amount of emotional support, this raises questions as to the vulnerability of elderly men as compared to elderly women. In support of this concern is the finding that women were more apt to name a confidant suggesting that even though men feel close to male friends or others they listed, they do not confide in or feel emotional support from them. It is their wives who function in this capadty. The obvious concern then is what happens to these men when their wives die or leave them. Given that virtually none of the present sample of males were widowers, this question could not be addressed with the data available. Clearly one goal of future research on the able elderly would be to examine more closely the lives of widowed males. The fact that little relationship between siblings, emotional support and psychological well-being was found in this research is somewhat puzzling given other data by such researchers as Cidrelli (1989). This difference suggests that the role of brothers and sisters may be overshadowed by others in the network, mainly children and female friends. Future studies may be more informative if more detailed questions regarding satisfaction with support were included in questionnaires or interviews. 69 Finally, the virtual lack of depression or other signs of psychological distress in this sample of able elderly is very encouraging. Given that these individuals function independently, drive cars, leave their neighborhoods at least once per week, reach out to friends and family, and feel good about their overall health is a sign that aging in our society need not be problematic, and in fact can be a rewarding and fulfilling part of life. It was uplifting to meet the individuals who agreed to partidpate in this research project. Some of them rode their bicycles to the interview, they were animated, interested, and eager to partidpate and help. One former professor shared fasdnating anecdotes about his department at the University and what it was like to work there 40 to 50 years ago. One woman, who had a chronic, severe systemic illness, felt that her health had little or no impact on her ability to do things, and rated her health as better than most people her age. She was a single woman who looked up at me when she got to the question on the BDI that pertained to increased thoughts about death and said: "Well, of course, I think about death more often than I used to, but not because I worry about dying or because I‘m depressed, but because I want to get my life in order so that when I die my niece doesn't have a financial mess to straighten out." Then there was the woman who rode her bike to the interview and asked if there was an aerobics fitness program on campus for the elderly. As our life span increases, vital, active individuals such as these will increase in number. One crudal area of research, therefore, must be further investigation into what contributes to the "Good Life" for the able elderly. Clearly, perception of health, concerns about memory, educational background, and emotional support stand out in the present research as significant areas for further study. APPENDICES APPENDD( A 70 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY M ”70006“ QINIC ' ova-rum Of ”YMMV MST WC 0 masses 0 ”I.” W GI?) ”W Enclosed is a flyer describing ”Coping with.Aging" sponsored by the Michigan State University Psychological Clinic. This study will examine 'memory and mood in individuals 55 years of age or older. If you would be willing to participate in this research. please call 355-9564 and ask for Sharon Ruggles or the Aging Project. All adults 55 or older are eligible, so if you know of anyone who qualifies, please pass this information on. I: is not necessary to have been an employee of Hichigan State‘University to be a part of the study. Sincerely. Ann-Marie Schee:baum,‘H.A. Coordinator, Coping with Aging Project 14.5.11. Psychological Clinic ANS/sat Enclosure It." ' u e- MIN-snow Anus/Fayed Weed!) ham-“- 71 COPING -WITH AGING PROJECT 0 need 100 volunteers 0 55 or older . 0 need not be retired As part of a research project , the MSU Psychological Clinic is giving memory and mood evaluations to people over 55. The first 100 who volunteer will be accepted, and people can participate whether they are retired or not. Those who participate can find out how they compare to others in their age group. This can be helpful for those who may be worried about how well they are doing as they get older. For more information and scheduling, please call 355-9564 and ask for Aging Evaluation. APPENDIX B 72 RESEARCH CONSENT FORM Coping with Aging Project I hereby agree to participate in the assessment portion of the Coping with Aging Project conducted by the Hichigan State University Psychological Clinic. I understand that I will be interviewed and be administered a series of tests. These are designed to measure memory functioning. mood and aspects of daily living. I also understand that these interviews wdll be audiotaped. A short time later I will be given feedback on the results of these tests. If I require further assistance. I will be provided referrals to appropriate agencies. I understand that the interview and questionnaire results will be held in strict confidence. and that I will be completely anonymous. I understand that my participation in this study is completely voluntary and I have the right to drop out of the study at any time. Hy right to further referral and services from the Clinic will not be affected by my decision to drop out of the study. Name Bite REFERENCES REFERENCES Albert, M. S. (1981). Geriatric neuropsychology.1g1_rm.l_gf£Mgngfl MW 42, 835-850 Aldwin, C. M., Spiro, A., Levenson, M. R., 8: Bosse, R. (1989). Longitudinal findings from the normative aging study. 1. Does mental health change with age?P sygho Iggy and Ag’mg, 4(3), 295-306. American Psychiatric Association (APA). (1980). W statistical Wm). Washington. DC: American Psychiatric Association. Antonucci, T. C. G: Akiyama, H. (1987). An examination of sex differences 1n social support among older men and women. W 1101/ 12), 737- 749. Arling, G. (1976). The elderly widow and her family, neighbors, and friends. lgurnal of Marriage; and the Family, 3_8_, 757-768. Babchuk, N. (1978-79). Aging and primary relations. Intgrnag'gngl lgurn algf Wm 2(2)137-151. Beck, A. T. (1967). Demign: clinicall exgrimental and theoretical asm. New York: Harper & Row. Beck, A.T., Ward, C. H, Mendelson,M., Mock, J., &Erbaugh, I. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Argygs gf angral Psyghiagy“, 4 53-63. Berg, 5., Mellstrom, D., & Persson, G. (1981). Loneliness in the Swedish aged. lgumal of Gerontology, &, 342-349. Blazer, D. 6: Williams, C. D. (1980). Epidemiology of dysphoria and depression in the elderly population. MW £714), 439-444. Blazer, D. G. (1983). Impact of late-life depression on the social network. Ameisaaloumalntfiwhiamb 149(2),162-166. Blazer, D., Hughes, D. C., & George, L. K. (1987). The epidemiology of depression in an elderly community population. The Gerontological men of America, 21(3), 281-287. Breslow, R., Kocs1s, I & Belkin, B. (1981). The contribution of the depressive perspective to memory function in depression. American Igurnal of 13mm 1&3, 227-230 73 74 Cavanaugh, S. & Wettstein, R. M. (1983). The relationship between severity of depression, cognitive dysfunction and age in medical inpatients. American lournal of Psychiagy,__,140 495. Cicirelli, V. G. (1982). Sibling influence throughout the lifespan. In M. E. Lamb & B. Sutton-Smith (Eds. ), Sibling Relationships. Thgir ngtgg and We litespan (pp. 267-284). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cicirelli, V. G. (1989). Feelings of attachment to siblings and well-being in later life. Echology and Agg’ g, 3(2), 211-216. Ciocon,I. 0. dc Patten]. F. (1988). Age-related changes 111 human memory. Normal and abnormal. 93:23:32. 43(10), 43-48. Cohen, R. M., Weingartner, H., Smallberg, S. A., Pickar, D., 6: Murphy, D. L. (1982). Effort and cognition in depression. Archives of General rim :9, 593-597. Crook, T. H. Gr Larrabee, G. I. (1990). A self-rating scale for evaluating memory in everyday life. Psycholggy and Agm' g, §(1), 48-57. Depner, C. E. 6: Ingersoll-Dayton, B. (1988). Supportive relationships in later life. WW 3(4). 348-357. Derogatis, L. R. (1977) WWW procedures for the SCL- 90. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Clinical Psychometrics Unit. Derogatis, L. R., Li an, R. 5., Covi, L., Rickels, K, 8: Uhlenhuth, E. R. (1970). Dimensions 0 outpatient neurotic patholo :Comparison of a clinical versus an empirical assessment. 9414M mm :2. 164-171. Derogatis, L. R. 8: Spencer, P. M. (1983). B51 Manual I: Administradgn and W. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Clinical Psychometrics Unit. DeWit, D. J., Wister, A. V., & Burch, T. K. (1988). Physical distance and social contact between elders and their adult children. Research on Aging, 19(1), 56-80. Esser, S. R. 6: Vitaliano, P. P. (1988). Depression, dementia, and social suppogséaomltgmadg' ngl Iddmg gf Agm' g gd Human Mldpment, Gallagher, D., Neis, 6., Gt Thompson, L. W. (1982). Reliability of the Beck Depression Inventory with older adults. W W 59(1) 152-153 7 5 Gallagher, D., Breckenbridge, J., Steinmetz, J., 8: Thompson, L. (1983). The Beck Depression Inventory and Research Diagnostic criteria: Congruence m an older population. lournal of Consulting and Clinical new 51(6) 945946 Gass, C. S. & Russell, E. W. (1986). Differential impact of brain damage and depression on memory test performance. MW Message» 52. 261-263 Gatz, M. 8: Hurwicz, M. (1990). Are old people more depressed? Cross- sectional data on center for epidemiological studies depression scale factors. _sxshelozxandm 5(2), 284-290 Hagestad, G. O. (1987). Able elderly 1n the family context: Changes, chances, and challenges. The Qerontglggical Society of America, 21(4), 417-422. Hale, W. D., Cochran, C. D. ,,Hedgepeth B. E. (1984). Norms for the elderly on the Brief Symptom Inventory WWW Exclaim 52(2). 321-322 Israel, B. A. (1982). Social networks and health status: Linking theory, research and practice. Patient Counseling and Health Education, g2), 65- 79. Kahn, R. L., Zarit, S. H., Hilbert, N. M., 6: Niederhe, G. (1975). Memory complaints and impairment in the aged: The effects of depression and altered brain functionAr Argv vgs of General Psychiady, 3_, 1569-1573. Kimmel, D. C. (1988). Ageism, psychology, and public policy. m Psychologist. 43(3). 175178. Krause, N. (1987). Satisfaction with social sup rt and self-rated health in older adults. Thg Qerontglggigal Society 0 America, _2_7(3), 301-308. LaRue, A., Dessonville, C., 6t Jarvik, L. F. (1985). Aging and mental disorders. In]. Birren&K.W.Schaie(Edsu)H§m1M1£9_flL¢_EW pp. 664-702. Lawton, M. P., Moss, M., Fulcomer, M., 8: Kleban, M. H. (1982). A research and service oriented multilevel assessment instrument. loumal of 9mm 12(1). 91-99. Lawton, M P. (1986). Functional assessment. In L. Teri & P. Lewinsohn (Eds) W New York: Springer Lee, G. R. & Ellithorpe, E. (1982). Intergenerational exchange and subjective well-being among the elderly. lgdmal Qf Marriaggan and 1115:le y, 4_,4U 217-224. 7 6 Lee, G. R. 8: Ishii-Kuntz. (1987). Social interactions, loneliness, and emotional well-being among the elderly. Wang, 2(4), 459- 482. Longino, C. F. 8c Lipman A. (1981). Married and spouseless men and women in planned retirement communities: Support network differentials. We. 43. 169-177 McAllister, T. W. 8: Price, T. R. P. (1982). Severe depressive pseudodementia with and without dementia. American lournal of ngchiap'y, 1&6), 626- 629. Murphy, E. (1982). Social origins of depression in old age. Brit_i§h lpumal pf menu 141. 135-142 Murphy, E. (1983). The prognosis of depression in old age. British lpurnal of chhiagy, 1_4_2_, 111-119. Murrell, S. A. Himmelfarb, S., 8: Wright, K. (1983). Prevalence of d ression and its correlates 1n older adults. Amggcan lpurnal pf Epi 11mdg mid ggy, _1__,17 173. Myers, I. K., Weissman, M. M., Tischler, G. L., Holzer, C. E., Leaf, P. I., Ovaschel, H., Anthony,I. C. Boyd, I. H., Burke, I. D., Kramer,M., 8: Stoltzman, R. (1984) Six-month prevalence of psychiatric disorders in three communities: 1980-1982. WM g, 959- 967. Newmann, I. P. (1989). Aging and depression. Psychology and Ag’ g, 4(2), 150-165. Perlmutter, M. (1978). What is memory aging the aging of? W melon 1.4. 330345. Powers, E. A. 8: Bultema, G. L. (1976). Sex differences 1n intimate friendships of old age We 38. 739-747 Preston, D. 8: G1imes, I. (1987). A study of difference in social support. lournal pf Qerontplpgical nging, _1_3_(2), 36-40. Revicki, D. A. 8: Mitchell, I. (1986). Social support factor structure in the elderlY- W 8(2). 232-248. Ronsman, K. M. (1988, Ianuary/ February). Pseudodementia. gm Nu_r§ing, pp. 50-52. Rozzini, R., Bianchetti, A., Carabellese, C., Inzoli, M., 8: Trabucchi, M. (1988). Depression, life events and somatic symptoms. The Gerdntplggigg w 28(2) 229-232 77 Ryan, M C. 8: Patterson, I. (1987). Loneliness in the elderly. W Gerontolpgigal Nursing, 13(5), 6-12. Salzman, C. 8: Gutfreund, M. I. (1987). Clinical techniques and research strategies for studying depression and memory. In L. W. Poon (Ed.), BMW. Washington, DC. APA Press. Schaie, K. W. (1988). Ageism in psychological research. American Psychplpggg' t, £6), 179-183. Shanas, E. (1979). Social myth as h ypoztheses The case of the family relations of old pe0ple W ypo12(1) 3-9 Shanas, E. (1980). Older people and their families: The new pioneers. Iournal of Marriagg and dge Family, 4;, 9-15. Small, G. W. 8: Jarvik, L. F. (1982). The dementia syndrome. Met, 1443-1446. Spreitzer, E. 8: Snyder, E. (1974). Correlates of life satisfaction among the aged. lournal of ficntology,_, 29 454-458. Storandt, M., Botwinick, I., Danziger, W. L., Berg, L., 8: Hughes, C. P. (1984). Psychometric differentiation of mild dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Archives of leogy, fl, 497-499. Storandt, M., Botwinick, I., Danziger, W. L. (1987). Longitudinal changes. Patients with mild SDAT and matched healthy controls. In L. Poon (ed) . 1 f _ - 1, Stroller, E., (1984). Self-assessments of health by the elderly: The importance of informal assistance. lournal of Health and Social Belgvior, 25, 260- 270. Surtees, P. G. (1980). Social su p,port residual adversity and depressive outcome My, 15. 71-80 Vaux, A. (1985). Variations 1n social su pport associated with gender, ethnicity. and age W40). 89-110 Ward, R A. (1985). Informal networks and well-being in later life: A research agenda. Thg Eontplpgical meg of America, §(1),55-61. Wechsler, D. 8: Stone, C. P. (1983). Manual: Wechsler Memogy Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation. 78 Williams,I. M., little, M. M., Scates, S., 8: Blockman, N. (1987). Memory complaints and abilities among depressed older adults. lgmal pf Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 5_5(4), 595-598. Wood, V. 8: Robertson, I F. (1978). Friendship and kinship interaction. Differential effects on the morale of the elderly. ldurnal pr mggg Qd mantel» 19. 367-375 Yesava e, I A. (1987). The use of self-rating depression scales 1n the elderly. In8 L. W. Poon (Ed), Handbook for clinical memogy assessment of older adults. Washington, D. C. APA Press, 213-217. Zarit, S. H. (1982). Affective correlates of self-reports about memory of older people WW mm 1(2). 25-35 Zung, W. K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of Gengal Psychiag, 1;, 63-70.