.>.!V .f... . nix! .Sihl . p..;.t 1.....V...... 1‘9; 51.}. . . I 3. .... .:... 1.. .. E V $99l§1 ‘.Xb..l :...:.~.v~.> . 7.. Av 9i._u..:......... . 2. .I.I.. .0 Ti... . .x l. w . 1.1.1.... . 7V VI-.. I.!. . THESlS VERS SITY LIB BRIAR ES lHIIIHlllllllllllllllllllllllll ”NW | 3 1293 00892 7448 This is to certify that the thesis entitled Architectural Privacy and Performance in Communication Therapy Settings for Head-Injured Patients: An Assessment of Speech-Language Pathologists' Perceptions presented by Diane Marie Sowash has been accepted towards fulfillment I of the requirements for MA degree in HED fl Hag/Z Major pi/fessord Datiflflwflétjé /7?5 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE W 9 m. V 5 MAY 3 1 2004 ll —_l MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution cMcMomit-o.‘ ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY AND PERFORMANCE IN COMMUNICATION THERAPY SETTINGS FOR HEAD-INJURED PATIENTS: AN ASSESSMENT OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGISTS’ PERCEPTIONS BY Diane Marie Sowash A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Human Environment and Design 1993 ABSTRACT ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY AND PERFORMANCE IN COMMUNICATION THERAPY SETTINGS FOR HEAD-INJURED PATIENTS: AN ASSESSMENT OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGISTS’ PERCEPTIONS BY Diane Marie Sowash If the interaction of environmental variables and patient variables have an impact on the effects of remedial efforts in head—injury rehabilitation programs, settings in which treatments occur may have an impact on patients' behavior. This exploratory study focused on a single environmental pattern--architectural privacy--and its perceived impact on the performance of speech language-pathologists and head- injured.patients in the beginning and final stages of therapy. A survey was used to gather qualitative and quantitive data from speech-language pathologists employed in a random sample of rehabilitation facilities for head-injured patients. Descriptive statistics provided profiles of therapists, patients, settings, architectural privacy, and performance. The Spearman rank correlation, measured the strengths of the relationships between architectural privacy and therapists’ and patients’ performances. Significant relationships were found to exist between architectural privacy and performance of speech-language therapists and head-injured patients in the beginning and final stages of therapy. The results support the notion that environmental inputs may have an influence on performance. DEDICATION This thesis is dedicated to my husband Larry, and son Mike, for their love and support in what at times seemed to be a project without end. iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to thank the following faculty in the Department of Human Environment and Design who served as my graduate committee. Without their valuable input and guidance this project would not have been possible. Roberta Kilty-Padgett, Associate Professor, Graduate Committee Chair. Timothy Springer, Ph.D., Professor, Department Chair. Susan Mireley, Ph.D., Associate Professor. Richard Graham, Associate Professor. I also wish to thank Charles Reeder, Ph.D., and Sandra Reeder of Tamarack Clinic, whose design project aroused my interest in the study of rehabilitation settings. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER PAGE I. II. DESCRIPTION OF THE PROBLEM Introduction........................................1 Statement of the Problem.............. ...... ........3 Purpose of Study ....... ...... ...... . ..... ...........4 REVIEW OF LITERATURE Environment and Behavior People-Environment Relationship...... ........ ..7 Behavior/Environment Congruence................8 Environmental Fit.............................1O Components of Stress in the Environment.......11 Person-Environment Compatibility..............12 Environmental Support or Hindrance............13 Environment as a Prosthetic Device............13 Privacy Meanings for Various Groups...................14 Behavioral Mechanisms.........................15 Physical and Psychological Privacy............16 Architectural Factors and Privacy.............17 Privacy in Communication Therapy Settings Control of Noise and Visual Distraction.......18 CHAPTER PAGE Need for Quiet and Noisy Environments.........19 Privacy in Educational Settings Impact of Noise on Performance................19 Privacy in Work Settings Providing Adequate Levels of Privacy..........20 Acoustical and Visual Privacy.. ........... ....21 Effects of Visual Distractions................22 Psychological Privacy .................. .......22 III. RESEARCH OBJECTIVES IV. Research Hypotheses................................24 Conceptual Model .............. ....... ............ ..25 Conceptual and Operational Definitions ..... ........27 METHODOLOGY Research Design and Procedures Design of the Study...........................31 Sample Selection... ........................... 32 Procedure.....................................32 Instrument Description........................33 Response Rate.................................34 Data Analysis Personal Characteristics......................35 Architectural Privacy.........................36 Relationships Between Type and Amount of Architectural Privacy....................37 Therapy Setting......... ..... .................38 vi CHAPTER PAGE Performance of Therapists and Patients........38 Relationships Between Privacy and Performance...OOOOOOOOOOOOOCCOOO000......39 Analysis of Variance..........................40 LimitationSCOOOC0....0.0.0.0000...0.00.00.00.00000041 Assumptions ...... O...0..0....0.0.0.000000000000000043 V. RESULTS Personal Characteristics Personal Characteristics of Speech-Language PathOlogistSOOOO0.0.0.0....00.0.00000000044 Personal Characteristics of Head-Injured Patients...00.00.0000...0.00000000000000045 Architectural Privacy Type of Architectural Privacy.................50 Amount of Architectural Privacy...............52 Relationships Between Type and Amount of Architectural Privacy....................53 Therapy Setting.OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOIO ..... 0.0.0....54 Performance of Therapists and Patients Concentration and Communication...............57 Effects of Visual and Acoustical Conditions...59 Relationships Between Privacy and Performance......64 Relationships Between Components of Architectural Privacy and Performance of Speech-Language Pathologists..........64 Relationships Between Components of Architectural Privacy and Performance of Head-Injured Patients in the Beginning Stages of Therapy..............67 vii CHAPTER PAGE Relationships Between Components of Architectural Privacy and Performance of Head-Injured Patients in the Final Stages of Therapy..................70 Relationships Between Architectural Privacy and Performance as Applied to the Research Hypotheses....... ..... ...74 Additional Relationships..... ........... ......76 Analysis of Variance ..... .... ................ ......76 Summary of Results Personal Characteristics of Speech-Language PathOIgists ..... O0.00.0.0...0.0.0.000000079 Personal Characteristics of Head-Injured PatientSOO0.0......0.00.00.00.0000000000079 Architectural Privacy.........................80 Relationships Between Type and Amount of Architectural Privacy............... ..... 80 Therapy Setting...............................81 Performance of Speech-Language Pathologists...81 Performance of Head-Injured Patients in the Beginning Stages of Therapy.. ........ 82 Performance of Head-Injured Patients in the Final Stages of Therapy ....... . ...... 82 Relationships Between Architectural Privacy and Performance .................... . ..... 83 Additional Relationships......................87 Analysis of Variance..........................87 VI. DISCUSSION Implications of the Findings Inferential Analyses..........................89 viii CHAPTER PAGE Descriptive Analyses..........................92 Generalizability and Application. ......... ....94 Directions for Future Research.....................94 Future Research Goals..............................95 Conclusion ..................... . ..... ..............96 APPENDICES A. UCHRIS Approval LetterOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO000......97 B. Pre-Test Cover Letter.........................98 C. Survey Cover Letter ........................ ...99 D 0 survey 0 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 100 REFERENCES ........................... . ................. 106 ix 10. 11. 12. 13. LIST OF TABLES EEGE Collapsed Age Distribution of Head-Injured Patients..46 Abilities of Head-Injured Patients at the Beginning and Completion of Therapy.............49 Office Type and Adjacency of Workspaces..............51 Maximum Numbers of Persons Sharing a Workspace.......51 Speech-Language Pathologists’ Awareness of Visual and Acoustical Conditions Beyond Personal workspace.0....0....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO ...... 53 Correlation of Type and Amount of Architectural Privacy ........... 0..0.0.0.......OOOOOOIOOOOOOOOSS Abilities of Head—Injured Patients and Speech- Language Pathologists to Concentrate and Communicate in Workspaces................ ...... .58 Effects of Visual and Acoustical Conditions on Therapists’ Abilities to Conduct Therapy........60 Effects of Visual and Acoustical Conditions on Patients’ Abilities to Participate in Therapy...63 Correlation of Components of Speech-Language Pathologists’ Performance and Type of Architectural Privacy. ..... .............. ..... ..65 Correlation of Components of Speech-Language Pathologists’ Performance and Amount of Architectural Privacy...........................67 Correlation of Components of Patient Performance in the Beginning Stages of Therapy and Type of Architectural Privacy........................68 Correlation of Components of Patient Performance in the Beginning Stages of Therapy and Amount of Architectural Privacy........................70 TABLE PAGE 14. Correlations of Components of Patient Performance in the Final Stages of Therapy and Type of Architectural Privacy........................71 15. Correlations of Components of Patient Performance in the Fianl Stages of Therapy and Amount of Architectural Privacy........................73 16. Correlation of Composite Scores for Performance and Privacy..OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO..0.0.0.0.00076 17. P-Values for Kruskal-Wallis Analysis of Variance of Performance Scores According to Architectural Privacy................... ...... ..78 18. Low and Moderate Correlations of Components of Performance and Type of Architectural Privacy...84 19. Low and Moderate Correlations of Components of Performance and Amount of Architectural Privacy.0.00.0.0......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO ...... 0.0.85 xi LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE IEGE 1. Model Representing Therapist’s Perception of the Relationship Between an Environmental Pattern, Personal Characteristics, and Performance of Therapists and Patients.........26 2. Age Distribution of Head-Injured Patients by quartiIESOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.0.0.00000000047 xii CHAPTER I. DESCRIPTION OF THE PROBLEM Introduction The 1980’s, according to Dixon, G011, and Stanton (1988), was the "decade of head injury" in the rehabilitation field. Increasing survival rates of head-injury 'victims due ‘to advances in medical technology, as well as the complexity of both the disability' and recovery' patterns, have «greatly increased the number of care models and head injury rehabilitation programs. Head-injured patients are persons who have received traumatic brain injuries as a result of penetrating or nonpenetrating head injuries. The size of this disability group is considerable: those surviving moderate to severe injuries number approximately 1.8 million, and those with mild head injuries total up to seven times that figure (Dixon et al., 1988). Two-thirds of head injury victims are under the age of 35, with men outnumbering women 2 to 1. A large number of head-injured patients are adolescents and young adults with relatively normal life expectancies who have survived motor vehicle accidents. Other causes of injuries are falls, sports injuries, tumors, strokes, and 'temporary interruption .90 very high correlation, very dependable relationship. 55 TABLE 6. Correlation of Type and Amount of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE TYPE OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY COMPONENTS Wall Ceiling Floor Door Persons' Oflfia? AMOUNT Aw“ HVAC -.l83 .074 -.024 .251 .105 .215 Aw mus -.189 .224 .096 .352* .180 .123 Aw phn .066 .236 .071 .208 -.002 .228 Aw conv -.038 .374* .063 .252 -.037 .383* Aw spyd .046 .125 -.012 .303* .315* .587* Aw vis .090 .180 .041 .281* -.044 .260 *p<.05; critical value = .261. 'Persons: persons sharing workspace. bOffice: private office or adjacency of nearest workspace if office is not private.‘mwt aware of. Awareness is based on therapists’ awareness of variable components. dSpy:others watching or listening. workspace feature best liked by 22 (53.66%) of the 41 persons who responded to the first question. The ability to control the environment by opening or closing the workspace door was liked best by three persons (7.32%). Each mentioned the importance of having the option to increase or reduce distractions or stimuli as needed for therapy by opening or closing the door. Freedom from visual distractions was mentioned by one respondent. Another respondent recognized that distraction should be a part of the therapeutic environment ("functional distractability") since it must be dealt with as part of real-life situations. According to the comments made in an accompanying letter by the director of a 56 communication disorders facility "...for certain disability groups, the introduction of outside detractors [sic] are needed.to evaluate the status of the individual’s progress and use of compensatory strategies." Other items which were best liked, but not a part of the study, dealt with issues such as access to co-workers and materials, adequate space, lighting, and comfort. Of the 44 speech-language pathologists responding to the question concerning one thing they would like to change in their workspace, two said they would not change anything. Eleven therapists (25%), however, would reduce the noise coming into their offices from outside. Sources of the noise included an elevator, a nearby copy machine which had become a staff gathering place, conversations in the next room, telephone noise in an adjacent office, a paging system, and a nearby activities room. One therapist commented that treatment was done in the patient dining room. Housekeeping staff, the paging system, and.patients going to the deck to smoke created "a maximally disruptive environment." Another therapist felt that.windows should be added to some offices for the option of additional stimulation. A larger workspace was desired by twelve (27.27%) of the respondents, and three (6.82%) therapists would like to decrease the number of professionals sharing workspaces. Although not a part of this study, air flow and temperature control were mentioned by six (13.64%) speech- 57 language therapists as something they would like to change in their workspaces. One person noted that the temperature in the workspace has "more effect on staff and patients." Other desired changes had to do with the relocation of services and activities, improved lighting, and the addition of a computer and carpeting to the workspaces. Performance of Therapists and Patients Concentration and Communication All 46 of the survey respondents answered the questions regarding the concentration and communication abilities of head-injured.patients and speech-language pathologists (Table 7). When asked how well their patients were able to concentrate in the workspace in the beginning stages of therapy, 21.47% (n = 10) of the therapists indicated "very well", 54.35% (n = 25) selected the middle category "somewhat", and 23.91% said "not*well" 01==11). Concentration improved greatly in the final stages of therapy. The category "very well" was selected by 84.78% (n.= 39) of the therapists, with only 15.22% (n = 7) selecting "somewhat", and none selecting "not well". Therapists indicated that 23.91% (n = 11) of head-injured patients’ communication abilities in the beginning stages of therapy in the workspace were in the "very well" category. Over one-half (56.52%, n = 26) of the patients were "somewhat" 58 TABLE 7 . Abilities of Head-Injured Patients and Speech-Language Pathologists to Concentrate and Communicate in Workspaces PATIENTS’ ABILITIES NOT WELL SOMEWHAT VERY WELL Concentrate 1' 23.91% 54.35% 21.74% Concentrate 2b 15.22% 84.78% Communicate 1 19.57% 56.52% 23.91% Communicate 2 26.09% 73.91% THERAPISTS’ ABILITIES Concentrate 4.35% 26.09% 69.57% Communicate 2.17% 15.22% 82.61% Note: N== 46.‘1: abilities in beginning of stages of therapy. b2: abilities in final stages of therapy. able to communicate and 19.57% (n = 9) were not able to communicate well. Communication improved in the final stages of therapy to 73.91% (n = 34) for the "very well" designation, and 26.09% (n = 12) for "somewhat". As with the ability to concentrate in the final stages of therapy, no selections were made of the "not well" designation. Many speech-language pathologists (69.57%, n = 32) felt that they were able to concentrate "very well" in their workspace. A lesser amount (26.09%, n = 12) were able to concentrate "somewhat", and a minority (4.35%. n = 2) were not able to concentrate well. Therapists were able to communicate "very well" with their patients in their workspace according to 82.61% (n.= 38) 59 of the respondents, and "somewhat" according to 15.22% (n.= 7) of the respondents. One respondent was not able to communicate well. Effects of Visual and Acoustical Conditions Speech-language pathologists were requested to indicate on a 5-step scale the degree to which specified visual and acoustical conditions affected their abilities to conduct therapy in their workspaces. Using the same scale, they were also requested to indicate their' patients’ abilities to participate in the beginning and final stages of therapy under these conditions in their workspaces. Generally, therapists thought the specified environmental conditions had little to no effect on their abilities to conduct therapy (Table 8). While a few respondents indicated that.some of the conditions somewhat enhanced their abilities, no»one indicated that the conditions enhanced their abilities. Eighty percent of the respondents felt that air conditioner, fan, or furnace sounds had no effect. Similarly, background music (80.95%) and others listening to or watching them (79.55%) had no effect. Visibility of others working or passing by had no effect on 72.73% of the respondents. Telephones ringing and/or office equipment sounds, and telephone and other conversations had a slightly greater 60 TABLE 8. Effects of Visual and Acoustical Conditions on Therapists’ Abilities to Conduct Therapy SOMEWHAT HAS NO SOMEWHAT CONDITION HINDERS HINDERS EFFECT ENHANCES HVAC sounds‘ 15.56% 80.00% 4.44% Background musicc 14.29% 80.95% 4.76% Telephones ringing, office equipment‘ 4.44% 46.67% 48.89% Conversations‘ 4.44% 44.44% 51.11% Others watching or listening” 20.45% 79.55% Visibility of others” 27.27% 72.73% ‘N = 45.1W'= 44.°N = 42. impact on therapists’ abilities to conduct therapy. Telephone and other conversations had no effect on 51.11% of the respondents’ abilities and somewhat hindered the abilities of 44.44%. Likewise, 48.89% of respondents indicated.that ringing telephones and equipment sounds had no effect, while 46.67% thought these sounds somewhat hindered their abilities to conduct therapy. According to speech-language pathologists, visual and acoustical conditions had relatively little effect on the majority of head-injured patients’ abilities to participate in the beginning and final stages of therapy (Table 9). Air 61 conditioner, fan, or furnace sounds had.no effect on 71.22% of the patients’ abilities and somewhat hindered 22.22% of the patients’ abilities to participate in the beginning stages of therapy. Ability to participate in the final stages of therapy was somewhat hindered by these sounds in 10.87% of the cases, and not affected in 86.96% of the cases. Responses concerning background music had a similar outcome. In the beginning stages of therapy 19.51% of patients’ abilities to participate were somewhat hindered, while 75.61% were unaffected. The distribution shifts slightly in the final stages of therapy. Background music somewhat hindered 16.28% of the head-injured patients, but had no effect on 81.40% of the patients’ abilities to participate in therapy. Telephones ringing and/or office equipment sounds had a more negative effect in both stages of therapy. Although 40% of the patients were unaffected in the beginning stage, the abilities of 46.67% of the patients were somewhat hindered in this stage, and 13.33% were hindered. Telephone and equipment sounds still somewhat hindered 34.38% of patients’ abilities in the final stages of therapy, but had no effect on 63.04% of the head-injured patients’ abilities to participate in therapy. The effects of telephone and other conversations on patients’ abilities showed.the least.amount.of change from the beginning therapy stage to the final stage. In the beginning 62 stage of therapy these conversations somewhat hindered 44.44% of the patients, compared with an increase to 48.89% in the final stages. This was the only instance in*which there was an increase in the "somewhat hinders" category from the beginning stage to the final stage. The "has no effect" response was recorded for 42.22% of the head-injured patients in the beginning stages of therapy and for 48.89% of the patients in the final stages. As with the previous condition concerning telephone and equipment sounds, conversations hindered 13.33% of the patients in the beginning stages of therapy. The two primarily visual conditions had identical responses regarding their effects on patients’ abilities to participate in the beginning stages of therapy. Others watching or listening, and visibility of others working or passing, hindered 11.36% of the head-injured patients. Responses indicated that patients’ abilities were somewhat hindered by both conditions in 29.55% of the cases. The two conditions had no effect in 59.09% of the cases. There was a slight variation in the numbers regarding the effects of the visual conditions on patients’ abilities in the final stages of therapy. Others watching or listening somewhat hindered 23.91% of the patients but had no effect on 73.91% of the patients. Visibility of others working or passing by somewhat hindered 22.22% and had no effect on 73.33% of patients’ abilities to participate in the final stages of therapy. 63 None of the speech-language pathologists indicated that any of the specified conditions enhanced their patients’ abilities to participate in either the beginning or final stages of therapy. One respondent indicated that all of the conditions somewhat enhanced his/her patients' abilities in the final stages of therapy. TABLE 9. Effects of Visual and Acoustical Conditions on Patients’ Abilities to Participate in Therapy SOMEWHAT HAS NO SOMEWHAT CONDITION HINDERS HINDERS EFFECT ENHANCES HVAC sounds l“ 6.67% 22.22% 71.22% HVAC sounds 2bc 10.87% 86.96% 2.17% Background music 18 4.88% 19.51% 75.61% Background music 2f 16.28% 81.40% 2.33% Telephones ringing, office equipment 1d 13.33% 46.67% 40.00% Telephones ringing, office equipment 2c 34.78% 63.04% 2.17% Conversations 1d 13.33% 44.44% 42.22% Conversations 2d 48.89% 48.89% 2.22% Others watching or listening 1c 11.36% 29.55% 59.09% Others watching or listening 2c 23.91% 73.91% 2.17% Visibility of others 1° 11.36% 29.55% 59.09% Visibility of others 2d 2.22% 22.22% 73.33% 2.22% ‘1: beginning stages of therapy.‘?: final stages of therapy. °N=46. “N= 45. °N= 44. fN = 43. 3N 41. 64 Relationships Between Privacy and Performance As previously stated, the primary goal of this research was to discover if, based on the perceptions of speech- language pathologists in rehabilitation settings, there are statistically significant relationships between architectural privacy and the performance of speech-language pathologists and head-injured patients. A nonparametric statistical test, the Spearman rank correlation (rhflg, was used to evaluate the relationships. Relarionships Between Components of Architectural Privacy ang Pgrfprnance of Speech-Language Pgtnologisrs Analysis of the relationships between the six individual components of type of architectural privacy and the eight performance components of speech-language pathologists (Table 10) indicated moderate correlations for the following pairs: a) ability to "concentrate" in workspace and type or.adjacency of nearest workspace (designated as "office" in tables), b) ability to "communicate" with patients in workspace and type or adjacency of nearest workspace, and c) others watching or listening ("watch/listen") and type or adjacency of nearest workspace. These relationships may be considered substantial when the correlation coefficient is between .40 and .70 (Williams, 1986). Low correlations (.261-.40 for the study), indicative of definite but small relationships, were found mainly in the pairs which included the availability of a 65 workspace "door" that can be closed, the number of "persons" who work in the space at the same time, and "office" type or adjacency. TABLE 10. Correlation of Components of Speech-Language Pathologists’ Performance and Type of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE TYPE OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY COMPONENTS Wall Ceiling Floor Door Persons‘ Ofifia? PERFORMANCEc Concentrate .223 .161 .005 .319* .339* .478* Communicate .164 .223 -.043 .340* .338* .454* HVAC sounds .081 .186 .027 .316* .191 .312* Music .080 .239 -.082 .370* .035 .301* Phones/equip .367* .177 .029 .144 -.082 .371* Conversation .202 .203 -.067 .151 .028 .226 Watch/listen .187 .230 .242 .323* .308* .696* Visibility .226 .300* .013 .263* -.016 .377* *p<.05; critical value = .261. ‘Persons: persons sharing workspace. bOffice: private office or adjacency of nearest workspace if office is not private.‘Performance: the ability to concentrate and communicate in the workspace, and the ability to conduct therapy under the specified conditions. Relationships between the six individual components of amount of architectural privacy and performance of speech- language pathologists were analyzed (Table 11). It was found that there were moderate correlations (.40-.70) between 8 pairs and low correlations (.261-.40) between 23 pairs of the 66 components. Moderate correlations (substantial relationships) were found to exist between therapists’ awareness of others watching or listening ("aw spy") and a) ability to "concentrate", b) ability 'to "communicate", and c) HVAC sounds; and between therapists’ awareness of visibility of others working or passing by ("aw vis") and ability to "concentrate" . Additional moderate correlations were indicated between similar’ awareness components and. performance components (eg., awareness of HVAC sounds ["aw HVAC"] and ability to conduct therapy under the condition of "HVAC sounds", etc.). A moderate correlation was also found between awareness of others watching or listening ("aw spy") and "HVAC sounds". Most of the low correlations (definite but small relationships) were between pairs which included the following privacy components: a) telephone and equipment sounds ("aw phn"), b) telephone and other conversations ("aw conv"), c) others watching and listening ("aw spy"), and d) visibiilty of others working or passing by ("aw vis"). Pairs involving awareness of background music ("aw'mus") and awareness of HVAC sounds ("aw HVAC") had more slight correlations (coefficient <.261), compared to the other privacy components (seven for "aw HVAC", and four for "aw mus"). Seventeen of the total 48 pairs had a correlation coefficient below the critical value of .261. 67 TABLE 11. . Correlation of Components of Speech-Language Pathologists ’ Performance and Amount of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE AMOUNT OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY . COMPONENTS Aw‘ HVAC Aw mus Aw phn Aw conv Aw spy” Aw v1s PERFORMANCE Concentrate .169 .270* .378* .261* .604* .398* Communicate .159 .285* .263* .065 .451* .159 HVAC sounds .393* -.093 .348* .337* .537* .290* Music .046 .398* .369* .378* .350* .278* Phones/equip .045 .083 .405* .227 .271* .173 Conversation .085 .366* .458* .513* .344* .338* Watch/listen .124 .009 .225 .306* .598* .290* Visibility .063 -.003 .321* .304* .238 .600* *p<.05.‘Aw: aware Of.‘Spy: others watching or listening. gelationships Between Components Of Architectural Privacy and Performance Of Head-Injured Patients in the Beginning Stages Of Therapy Analysis of the relationships between components Of type Of architectural privacy and patients’ performance in the beginning stages Of therapy resulted in low or moderate correlations for 14 Of 48 pairs (Table 12). The four pairs which showed a moderate correlation, therefore exhibiting a substantial but small relationship, all had type or adjacency Of nearest workspace ("Office") as one component. The performance components of the pairs included ability tO participate in therapy under the conditions Of a) "HVAC 68 TABLE 12. . Correlation Of Components Of Patients’ Performance in the Beginning Stages Of Therapy and Type Of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE TYPE OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY COMPONENTS Wall Ceiling Floor Door Persons‘ Offim" PERFORMANCE° Concentrate —.090 .092 .066 .229 .041 .141 Communicate .093 .054 .012 .245 -.011 .191 HVAC sounds -.226 .148 .018 .319* .370* .469* Music .009 .281* .109 .381* -.022 .380* Phones/equip .167 .059 .124 .262* -.081 .219 Conversation .151 .140 .134 .262* .012 .518* Watch/listen .285* .172 .184 .284* .163 .597* Visibility .173 .053 .075 .284* .156 .542* *p<.05. ‘Persons: persons sharing workspace. bOffice: private Office or adjacency of nearest workspace if Office is not private. FPerformance: the ability to concentrate and communicate in the workspace, and the ability to participate in therapy under the specified conditions. sounds", b) telephone and other "conversations", c) others watching or listening ("watch/listen"), and.d) "visibility" Of others working or passing by. The availability Of a workspace door that can be closed was a component in 6 of the 10 pairs Of low correlations. "Door" had a definite but small relationship with head-injured patients’ abilities to participate in therapy under the conditions Of a) "HVAC sounds", b) background "music", c) 69 telephones ringing and/or other Office equipment sounds ("phones/equip"), d) telephone and other "conversations", e) others watching or listening ("watch/listen"), and f) "visibility" Of others working or passing by. The measurement Of the strengths Of the relationships between components Of amount Of architectural privacy and patients’ performance in the beginning stages Of therapy resulted in moderate correlations (.40-.70) for 6 pairs and low correlations for 12 pairs (Table 13). As with the correlations between therapists’ performance and amount Of architectural privacy, moderate correlations--four in this case--were found between ‘pairs Of similar awareness and performance components (eg., awareness Of the visibility Of others working or passing by ["aw vis"] and the ability to conduct therapy under the condition Of "visibility" Of others working or passing by). The two remaining moderate correlations were between a) awareness of others watching and listening ("aw spy") and telephone and other conversations ("conversation"), and b) "aw HVAC" and "conversation". Eleven of the 12 pairs having low correlations involved the amount Of architectural privacy components dealing with awareness Of telephone and equipment sounds, conversations, others watching or listening, and visibility Of others; and the performance components of background music, telephone and equipment sounds, conversations, others watching or listening, and visibility of others. 70 TABLE 13. Correlation Of Components Of Patients’ Performance in the Beginning Stages of Therapy and Amount Of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE AMOUNT OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY COMPONENTS Aw‘ HVAC Aw mus Aw phn Aw conv Aw spy” Aw vis PERFORMANCE Concentrate .165 .076 .080 .215 .111 .185 Communicate .091 -.008 .070 -.014 .019 .113 HVAC sounds .529* .151 .233 .203 .522* .136 Music .287* .447* .155 .378* .273* .235 Phones/equip .182 .121 .379* .196 .153 .126 Conversation .422* .167 .346* .310* .392* .317* Watch/listen .239 .025 .303* .336* .569* .379* Visibility .132 -.023 .210 .248 .300* .523* *p<.05. ‘Aw: aware of. Awareness is based on therapists’ awareness of variable components. bSpy: others watching or listening. Relationships Between Components Of Architectural Privacy and Perfprmance Of Head-Injured Patients in the Final Stages of Therapy Analysis Of the relationships between components Of type Of architectural privacy and patients’ performance in the final stages Of therapy resulted in low or moderate correlations for 10 Of 48 pairs (Table 14). The two pairs which showed moderate correlations--indicative Of a substantial but small relationship--had type or adjacency Of 71 TABLE 14. Correlation Of Components Of Patients’ Performance in the Final Stages of Therapy and Type Of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE TYPE OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY COMPONENTS Wall Ceiling Floor Door Persons‘ Office” PERFORMANCEc Concentrate -.043 .147 .154 .351* .254 .198 Communicate .053 .191 .018 .249 .098 .179 HVAC sounds -.314* -.008 -.096 .393* .139 .218 Music -.083 .177 .045 .359* -.027 .236 Phones/equip .241 -.097 -.006 .208 .043 .486* Conversation .045 .035 -.186 .156 -.000 .452* Watch/listen .177 -.041 .085 -.072 .137 .396* Visibility .181 .052 -.193 -.074 -.150 .317* *p<.05.‘Persons: persons sharing workspace. I’Office: private Office or adjacency of nearest workspace if Office is not private.‘Terformance is based on the ability tO concentrate and communicate in the workspace as well as the ability to participate in therapy under the specified visual and acoustical conditions. nearest workspace ("Office") as one component. The performancecomponents Of the pairs included ability to participate in therapy under the conditions of a) telephones ringing and/or other office equipment sounds, and.b) telephone and other "conversations". "Door" had a definite but small relationship (low correlation) with head-injured patients’ abilities to "concentrate" in the therapists’ workspaces, and to 72 participate in therapy under the conditions Of "HVAC sounds" and background "music". A low correlation was also evident between type or adjacency Of nearest workspace ("Office") and and a) "visibility" Of others working or passing by, and b) others watching or listening ("watch/listen"). A negative low correlation was found between "wall" surface and "HVAC sounds". The strengths of the relationships between components Of amount Of architectural privacy and patients’ performance in the final stages of therapy can be seen in Table 15. Moderate correlations occurred for 5 pairs and low correlations for 17 pairs. Two moderate correlations were found between pairs Of similar awareness and performance components: a) awareness Of telephone and other conversations ("aw conv“) and the ability tO conduct therapy under the condition Of telephone and other "conversations", and b) awareness Of visibility ("aw vis") and the ability tO conduct therapy under the condition Of "visibility" Of others working or passing by. Substantial relationships (moderate correlations) were shown to exist between the two remaining pairs: a) the correlation between awareness of others watching and listening ("aw spy") and telephones ringing and/or Office equipment sounds ("phones/equip"), and b) the correlation between awareness Of visibility Of others working or passing by ("aw vis") and telephone and other "conversations". 73 TABLE 15. Correlation Of Components Of Patients’ Performance in the Final Stages of Therapy and Amount Of Architectural Privacy VARIABLE AMOUNT OF ARCHITECTURAL PRIVACY COMPONENTS Aw‘ HVAC Aw mus Aw phn Aw conv Aw spyb Aw vis PERFORMANCE Concentrate .134 .327* .188 .334* .149 .092 Communicate .135 .118 .257 .229 .049 .238 HVAC sounds .267* .082 .251 .406* .325* .293* Music .105 .346* .106 .328* .286* .083 Phones/equip -.046 .049 .314* .283* .407* .371* Conversation .347* .209 .362* .433* .288* .451* Watch/listen -.021 -.254 .103 .061 .360* .174 Visibility -.055 -.286* .157 .283* .150 .438* *p<.05. ‘Aw: aware Of. Awareness is based on therapists’ awareness Of privacy components. l’Spy: others watching or listening. Low correlations, denoting definite but small relationships, were found between 17 pairs Of variable components. Telephone and other conversations ("aw'conv"), and others listening or watching ("aw spy") had the most low correlations (four) with performance variable components. Low indicated between awareness Of correlations also were background music ("aw mus") and three performance variables, one of which. was a .negative correlation. Three. privacy variables--awareness Of HVAC sounds ("aw HVAC"), awareness Of 74 telephones ringing and/or office equipment sounds ("aw phn"), and awareness Of visibility Of others working or passing by ("aw vis") --each had two definite but small relationships (low correlations) with performance variable components. Beleripnships Between Archirectual Erivacy ang Eerformance es Applieg to the Researcn Hypotheses In order to respond to the research hypotheses for this study, the composite scores for each variable3 were analyzed using the Spearman rank correlation (rm), a nonparametric test which measured the strength Of the relationships between each pair Of variables (Table 16). A p<.05 (one-tailed test) was required tO reject the null hypotheses Of nO relationships and accept the working hypotheses of the existence Of significant relationships between architectural privacy and performance Of speech-language pathologists and head-injured patients. The critical value for each test was .261 with N -5 = 41. Hypothesis 1: There is a significant relationship between architectural privacy and the performance of speech-language pathologists in rehabilitation settings. Results of the nonparametric statistical analysis indicated a moderate correlation (correlation coefficient 3Numerical values of the responses to questions about each variable were summed to arrive at a single composite score for the variable. 75 .560) between architectural privacy and performance Of speech- language pathologists. Since the relationship is considered significant when the critical value equals or exceeds .261 (p<.05, one-tailed test) the null hypothesis Of no relationship can be rejected. This moderate correlation is indicative Of a substantial relationship between the two variables. Hypothesis 2: There ia a significant relationship between architectural privacy and the performance of head-injured patients in the beginning stages .of therapy in rehabilitation settings. A moderate correlation (correlation coefficient .566) was found to exist between architectural privacy and performance Of head-injured patients in the beginning stages Of therapy. Since p<.05 (one-tailed test) the null hypothesis Of no relationship can be rejected. As in the previous hypothesis, there is a substantial relationship between the two variables. Hypothesis 3: There is a significant relationship between architectural privacy and the performance of head-injured patients in the final stages of therapy in rehabilitation settings. The correlation coefficient for the paired variables architectural privacy and performance Of head-injured patients in the final stages of therapy was .429. Although it is lower than those in Hypotheses 1 and 2, it is still above the critical value Of .261 necessary for p<.05 (one-tailed test), so the null hypothesis Of no relationship can be rejected. 76 TABLE 16. Correlation Of Composite Scores for Performance and Privacy Performance VARIABLE Therapist Patient 1‘ Emmet 2» Architectural Privacy .560* .566* .429* Therapist Performance -- .763* - .696* *p<.05. ‘Patient 1: patient in beginning stages of therapy. bPatient 2: patient in final stages Of therapy. Addirional Relationships An.additional finding Of the analysis was the strength.of the relationships between therapists’ performance and patients’ performance (Table 16). The correlation coefficient for the paired values of speech-language pathologists’ performance and head-injured patients’ performance in the beginning stages Of therapy was high (.763), pointing to a marked relationship between the pairs. A similar relationship (correlation coefficient .696), was found tO exist between speech-language pathologists’ performance and performance Of head-injured patients in the final stages Of therapy. Analysis Of Variance The Mann-Whitney two-sample median procedure was used to determine whether or not speech-language therapists’ perceptions of themselves and their patients differed 77 regarding performance. The composite performance scores for speech-language pathologists were compared with those Of head- injured patients in the beginning and final stages of therapy. A probability Of less than .05 (p<.05, one—tailed test) was required.to indicate that the medians of the two samples being compared were different. Results indicated that there was a difference between speech-language pathologists’ perceptions Of their performance and head-injured patients’ performance in the beginning stages Of therapy (.0001p<.05). Composite performance scores for head-injured patients in the beginning stages and in the final stages Of therapy were also compared. Results indicated that there was a difference between the two sample medians (.0000p<.05). Results indicated that there was a difference between 88 therapists’ perceptions Of patients’ performance in the beginning stages Of therapy and in the final stages Of therapy (.0000