.: 8 Huh. ~ - “K? E A4) A 9;, 11:» ' 5 K e f— “y‘iL -: ;;- . w .m " u .3, . 3‘15”}, 7 .4 _ , :.":;&»{g‘ .. :m M; “ «‘93‘63. - 'T‘r-faiixtxp: 1 4 ‘91:“ n“. figgr‘.‘:l,_ fan: 3 :3; \“9 5‘4"}? 1‘5; 5.121434“? ‘EL ‘5‘ ""1 “‘"m' If? n. . . l' ‘ ‘ ws‘m'k- :v . '::re‘¥ai"""‘83~e (.‘X , w 44.»:32‘: ( ‘ ‘ 1nt1? “2-H." «*w- v. . .‘94‘ ”1 fl"- ‘ - $13 1‘4“"? x ¥II;U—" ‘ :‘m $22!:LX u f .1 Vii-1 .._ .‘v 2.3.144"; w '1‘ ”I154 ‘4‘ ‘\"I K . fi'wh‘nvi‘si. 7.77“".rm..<12‘ -‘3 Vr:1r In.“ W. ‘59,?“ :13“ . . 3. : (‘- A 4 an ‘m x . 1m ('4 :“W‘F’t :r-if‘VZfis. ém‘pfi é")? ‘ vhf. 5?? .a& «4 31:14.19: .., -~ ..-4 U; 1., u.- ‘= .‘u,. “75“ ““ " w". I.\'u WW; :7 & ~ «A -m,‘ a _ .. . . 1 4*, ~ v ,u l'n ‘ . “ '9} "w 45‘7; ' -’ ‘ 1 -4 I‘m ' “ 4 -r , -\4‘ - S» ., A .. . . . 4- s“ “1333?. 'U -u ..\ .4; m a . . an 014....34. A", , . ‘ ~ , ‘ (‘r_' *w r .44 .. ..._ . V r .. .1 4L “’43, PM '3 “- ”4.» < , - ‘ ‘ -- - my ’ i .1 ‘ '_ , ‘ .-4 y . 1‘ > ‘ w - .4 §v u. ‘ a . 4, ¢ \4 r x I ‘ . I ’ “ *5 - am. “£353.32: . ‘ (‘4‘ nq...‘\ ~\ 2" ‘. J .1“ .0 ..'< ' ‘ ' ‘ ‘/ 1 ‘31.: -:4 \‘ .r'u I n. ., -\4u'>.:4 4 .4 . , . ‘ v- P‘ - , .1.” ‘4 . - D " ' N ’4” Jan - 5k! . ., . . ’ ' - V ' t' 4 ‘ 4 . *t’i‘é . .4 4.. . mm m cr ‘va a“??? I 3 , ‘ ‘. .f‘. . \ . ‘4 wh'T-i‘ o . -h J n -.‘ r 2’ 4 i.‘ ". ,4 f Date 0-7 639 ICHGANS H HHHHIHHHHHH 57122 IH This is to certify that the thesis entitled GENETIC LOADING FOR ALCOHOLISM: NEW EVIDENCE FOR SUBTYPES presented by Deborah A. Wynblatt has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M .A. degree in Psychology I Major profe 5-11-90 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution '____.__.__—_ 7 ‘—-r—i ———_————- 7 ——— LIBRARY w Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE I MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution czkimMune-nt GENETIC LOADING FOR ALCOHOLISM: NEW EVIDENCE FOR SUBTYPES By Deborah Ann Wynblatt A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1990 . ,v‘ - (1 wt?“ “/11 ,/ ABSTRACT GENETIC LOADING FOR ALCOHOLIS M: NEW EVIDENCE FOR SUBTYPES By Deborah Ann Wynblatt Current research on the etiology of alcoholism suggests that genetic vulnerability plays a role in the development of alcoholism. This study, using a population-based sample and a broad set of independent variables, examined how genetic loading for alcoholism affects developmental course of alcoholism and what moderating effects various environmental variables might have on genetic load. Results confirmed that high genetic loading for alcoholism was related to earlier onset and greater severity of drinking difficulties and more frequent alcohol use. More importantly, alcoholic subtype was a significant factor in predicting etiologic pathways into alcoholism. Among Type 2 alcoholics, genetic loading for alcoholism contributed strongly to later alcohol problems, as did history of socialization to aggression. For Type ls, rearing in an alcoholic environment was the only predictor of adult alcohol problems, suggesting that socialization to using alcohol as a coping mechanism is most important for this subtype. To Tom iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am indebted to Robert Zucker for his guidance and support throughout the development of this work. His knowledge of the alcoholism literature as well as his theoretical conceptualization of the phenomenon were invaluable in broadening my understanding of and approach to this work. The other members of my thesis commitee, Frank Floyd and Ralph Levine, also deserve my thanks for their support and help. Frank Floyd in particular contributed a significant amount of time and energy to helping me with my data analyses; without his aid, my understanding of path analysis would have been much more simplistic. Finally, thanks go out to all the staff members of the M.S.U. family Project who so assiduously collected the data I used, as well as to the project families who provide us with so much information about their lives. This work was supported in part by grants to R. A. Zucker, R. B. Noll and H. E. Fitzgerald from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (AA 07065) and from the Michigan Department of Mental Health, Prevention Services Unit. iv W LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. vii LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................... viii CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION AND REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Alcoholism: A Generic Etiology? ........................................................................... 3 Genetics and Human Alcohol Consumption ........................................................ 5 Twin Studies ....................................................................................................... 5 Adoption Studies ............................................................................................... 9 What do Alcoholics Inherit? .................................................................................. l4 BiochemicalAbnormalities .......................................................................... 15 Brain Abnormalities ..................................................................................... 16 Temperament ................................................................................................. 18 Statement of the Problem ....................................................................................... 18 CHAPTERII: HYPOTHESES .............................................................................................. 21 CHAPTER III: METHODS Subjects ..................................................................................................................... 23 Data Collection ......................................................................................................... 25 Measures ....................................................................... 25 Alcoholic Subtype ............................................................................................. 25 Drinking Measures ........................................................................................... 28 Genograrns ......................................................................................................... 29 Antisocial Behavior ............................................................................... ' ........... 30 Socioeconomic Status (SES) .............................................................................. 33 V Environmental Exposure to Alcoholism ....................................................... 33 CHAPTER III: RESULTS Relationship Between Family Expression of Alcoholism and Drinking Variables ................................................................................................................... 35 Overall Relationships ....................................................................................... 35 Type 1 versus Type 2 Alcoholics .................................................................... 35 Relationship Between Lifetime Alcohol Problems Score and Other Life Difficulties ................................................................................................................ 4] Path Analyses ................................................................................................... 43 CHAPTER IV: DISCUSSION Influence of Other Variables on the Developmental Course of Alcoholism .............................................................................................................. 50 Future Directions .................................................................................................... 53 Summary .......................................................... ' ....................................................... 5 4 APPENDICES .................................................................................................................... 57 Appendix A: Full Models of Distal Predictors of LAPS for Type is and Type 28 ...................................................................................................................... 57 Appendix B: Demographic Questionnaire .......................................................... 58 Appendix C: Diagnostic Interview Schedule ................. .................................... 64 Appendix D: Drinking and Drug History Questionnaire .................................. 75 LIST OF REFERENCES ..................................................................................................... 88 vi W5 TABLE PAGE 1. Demographic Characterisrics of Subjects ...................................................... 24 2. Possible Family Illnesses .................................................................................. 26 3. Correlation Between Family Expression of Alcoholism and Alcohol Involvement for Men ......................................................................................... 36 4. Differences Between Type 1(n=25) and Type 2 (n=60) Alcoholics on Demographic/ Life Difficulty Variables ........................................................ 37 5. Correlation Between Family Expression of Alcoholism and Alcohol Involvement for Type 1 versus Type 2 Alcoholics ....................................... 39 6. Correlation Between Family Expression of Alcoholism and Alcohol Involvement for Type 1 versus Type 2 Alcoholics after Controlling for Parental Alcoholism in the Rearing Environment ............................... 4O 7. Correlations among Life Difficulty Variables for Type 1(n=25) and Type 2 (n=60) Alcoholic Men ............................................................................. 42 vii W FIGURE PAGE 1. Diagram for Classification of Alcoholic Subtype ......................................... 27 2. Coefficient of Relationship for Hypothetical Subject and His Extended Family ................................................................................................................... 31 3. Sample Calculation: Family Expression of Alcoholism ................................ 32 4. Comparison of Path Models for Type 1 and Type 2 Alcoholism ................ 45 5. Extended Path Model for Type 2 Alcoholics .................................................... 46 6. Full Models of Distal Predictors of LAPS for Type Is and Type 2s ............... 57 viii Introduction and Literature Review Research into the etiology of alcoholism has been hampered by the lack of a widely shared definition of alcoholism. Because of this lack of consistency, it is often difficult to compare research across studies; one cannot be sure that the same populations are under study. The classical definition of alcoholism was formulated by EM. Jellinek in the 1950's. He decried the broader medical profession's use of the word 'alcoholism' for any type of heavy drinking. Instead, Jellinek (1952) distinguished two types of alcoholics (addictive and non-addictive) in whom excessive drinking occured WW. Using the medical model, he applied the disease conception of alcohol addiction nor to excessive drinking per se, but to the 'loss of control' over drinking that accompanied it. He considered this loss of control to be a disease condition that only occured in addicrive alcoholics. More modern formulations of the medical model of alcoholism include the diagnostic criteria of DSM-IlI-R (A.P.A.,l987). As did Jellinek, DSM-III-R considers alcoholism to be a disease with some unknown etiology. It defines peOple with alcohol dependence syndrome as those who have any three of the following symptoms: a loss of control over alcohol intake, a desire to cut down on intake, a great deal of time spent obtaining alcohol, frequent intoxication or withdrawal symptoms which interfere with role obligations, participation in important social or occupational aetivities reduced due to alcohol use, continued use despite recognition of a problem with alcohol, tolerence of alcohol, withdrawal symptoms, or intake of alcohol to avoid withdrawal. Because alcoholism is thought to exist in differing degrees of severity, not all of these indicators need be present to define a person as an alcoholic. AnOther medical model of alcoholism which has recently been proposed is Edward and Gross's (1976) Alcohol Dependence Syndrome. The seven facets of the syndrome are narrowing of the drinking repetoire, salience of drink-seeking behavior, increased 2 tolerance to alcohol, repeated withdrawal symptoms, repeated relief of withdrawal symptoms by further drinking, subjective awareness of a compulsion to drink and reinstatement of the syndrome after abstinence. Edward and Gross' definition of alcoholism differs from DSM-III-R in that social problems resulting from drinking are nor included as indicators of alcohol dependence (Caetano, 1985). The term 'alcoholism' has been used differently in various studies; n0t all use the DSM-III-R criteria (or those of its predecessor DSM-III) to obtain samples of alcoholics. Bohman (1978), for example, defined his sample of alcoholics based on law-breaking: alcoholics were those who had been registered with local Temperence Boards set up to regulate misuse of alcohol. Defining alcoholism in such a manner is problematic, since criteria for being registered with the Temperence Board have changed over time. Cadoret, Troughton and O'Gorman (1987) considered alcoholics to be those who had one or more recorded social or medical problems because of drinking (i.e. lost a job due to drinking, got a divorce due to drinking). In similar fashion, Schuckit (1984) defined alcoholism as the occurence of one of several alcohol- related major life problems, such as dropping out of school. One problem with such definitions is that they do not clearly differentiate alcoholics from 'problem' drinkers, who may also have had difficulties functioning in society due to excessive drinking. One way to distinguish between these two groups may relate to Jellinek's (1952) original definition of alcoholism; 'problem' drinkers might well be conceptualized as Jellinek's 'non-addiCtive' alcoholics, who do n0t lose control over their drinking but may still drink to excess. Mulford and Miller (1960) described alcoholism as a function of a set of social norms which gave the individual a certain way of thinking about or defining himself, alcohol, and the relationship between the two. Alcoholic drinking, in Mulford and Miller's conceptualization, allowed an alcoholic to redefine himself and his relationship to Others in a way that was more self-satisfying. Unlike those discussed previously, this definition conceptualized alcoholism within a social learning theory framework. 3 One important question needs to be answered in order to produce a useful definition of alcoholism: is alcoholism one end of a continuum that includes 'heavy' or 'problem' drinkers? Goodwin (1979) has argued, based on his studies of Danish adoptees, that alcoholism results from a different developmental path than does 'heavy drinking'. However, research by Clark and Cahalan (1976) has indicated that more severe forms of alcoholism merge gradually with problem and heavy drinking. A distinct, pathological type of alcoholism does not appear to stand out on a population curve of those with drinking problems (Peele, 1986). 8 Finally, the issue of whether there are several different kinds of alcoholism, each with its own etiology, remains. Zucker (1987a) has proposed that four different alcoholisms, each with its own cause and course, exist: antisocial alcoholism, developmentally limited alcoholism, developmentally cumulative alcoholism and negative affect alcoholism. Cloninger and Bohman's (1981) research also points to the need to develop definitions of alcoholism which are specific to different alcoholic subpopulations. ”11"!5'5'1" What kinds of evidence led to the idea that inherited factors might play a role in the development of alcoholism? One of these was family studies. Such studies indicated that rates of alcoholism were much higher among the families of alcoholics than among the general population. Cotton's (1979) literature review encompases 39 studies of alcoholics. She found that regardless of the nature of the non-alcoholic population used as a comparison group, an alcoholic was more likely than a non-alcoholic to have a mother, father or other close relative who was alcoholic. The fact that alcoholics were twice as likely to report parental alcoholism as other psychiatric patients implied that a high rate of familial alcoholism was mm; to alcoholics. Two-thirds of the studies that Cotton reviewed found at least 25% of all alcoholics to have alcoholic fathers. One third of alcoholics were found to have at least one alcoholic 4 parent. Corton noted that if anything, her figures erred on the side of conservatism due to demonstrated underreporting of mental illness and alcoholism in the 39 studies. Family studies also indicate that the rate of alcoholism is increased in the offspring of alcoholics. Goodwin (1979) reported that sons of alcoholics were four times as likely as were sons of non-alcoholics to become alcoholics themselves. Winokur, Reich, Rirrrrner and Pitts (1970) also investigated the occurence of alcoholism in children of alcoholics. They diagnosed 31% of the sons of male alcoholics to be alcoholic upon reaching adulthood; an astonishing 51% of sons of female alcoholics were found to be alcoholic as adults. The fact that the rate of alcoholism is higher within families of alcoholics than in the general population does not by itself prove that a genetic vulnerability to alcoholism exists. An alternative explanation would be that similar environments are recreated within alcoholic families, such that offspring in each generation are exposed to an alcohologenic environment. But when coupled with results from animal research, the concept of a genetic role in the development of alcoholism begins to be a plausible hyporhesis. Animal studies have demonstrated strain and line differences in the acquisition of alcohol preference, sensitivity, tolerance and dependence. Building animal models for alcohol preference, for example, provides information about genetic mechanisms related to voluntary excessive consumption in humans. Lumeng, Hawkins and Li (1977) were able to use selective breeding to produce a true-breeding strain of rats that voluntarily consumed large amounts of alcohol. However, it could be argued that the rats were drinking the alcohol because they liked the taste or smell, rather than, like humans, for its mood altering effects. To demonstrate that this was not the case, Waller, McBride, Gatto, Lumeng and Li (1984) inserted devices directly into the stomachs of rats which provided the rats with their choice of water or alcohol. Rats bred from the alcohol-preferrin g strain self-infused with up to 9.4 g of alcohol per kilogram body weight each day as compared to the non- alcohol preferring strain of rats which self-infused a maximum of .7 g per kilogram body 5 weight per day. Apparently, the rats bred for alcohol preference were seeking the pharmacological effect of the alcohol. The fact that animals usually refuse to drink alcohol (Petrakis, 1985) makes this research even more intriguing. Genetics have also been shown to play a role in animals' sensitivity to alcohol. McCleam and Kakihana (1981) developed two strains of mice which they called short sleep (88) or long-sleep (LS) based on the duration of unconciousness after receiving a dose of ethanol. Selective breeding over several generations produced large differences in sensitiVity to alcohol. Quantities of ethanol that merely anesthetized SS mice were close to lethal for LS mice. Mice selected for their genetically lower sensitivity to alcohol have also been shown to develop alcohol tolerance more quickly during prolonged exposure to alcohol (Gallaher and Gionet, 1988). Lastly, two groups of researchers have developed animal models which demonstrate a genetic contribution to alcohol dependence in rrrice. Crabbe, Kosobud and Young (1983) and Allen, Petersen, Wilson and McCleam (1983) have selectively bred different strains of mice which can be distinguished on the basis of the severity of their withdrawal symptoms after alcohol is removed from their diet. Therefore, indirect research evidence points to a genetic contribution to the development of alcoholism in humans. More direct evidence from human genetic studies will now be reviewed. 5 . 1H 5] 11C . I. S 1' There are several kinds of studies which allow conclusions to be drawn about the role of inheritence in the development of alcoholism. One type is the twin study, where drinking behavior in monozygous (MZ) twins (who are assumed to be genetically identical) is compared to drinking behavior in dizygous (DZ) twins (who are related as normal 6 siblings). Higher rates of concordant drinking behavior in the monozygous twins can then be attributed to their more similar genotype. In one early twin study, Partanen, Brunn and Markannen (1966) investigated drinking behavior in twins with 'normal' drinking patterns. The only inclusion criteria for the study were that subjects be a member of a twin pair born in Finland between 1920 and 1929 and that they currently be living in Finland. After recruiting 902 male twin pairs, the group found a significantly higher concordance rate in amount and density (i.e. frequency) of drinking among MZ twins than among DZ twins. Heritability was calculated to be .36 for amount of drinking and .39 for density of drinking; Partanen et al. concluded that genetic factors influence drinking patterns among normal individuals. Similar research was conducted by Kaprio, Koshenvuo, Langinvanio, Romanov, Sarna and Rose (1987) who also assessed 'normal' drinking behavior. They compared 879 male pairs of monozygous twins and 1940 pairs of dizygous twins from the Finnish Twin Cohort. The Finnish Twin Cohort consists of all like-sex twin pairs born in Finland in 1958 among whom both twins were living in 1967. Subjects used in this study comprised nearly all surviving male twins in the 25-49 age range. Information was requested on the frequency (number of days of alcohol use), quantity (amount of alcohol used) and density (number of days of excessive use) of alcohol consumption per month and the frequency of passouts in the previous year. Results confirmed genetic effects for frequency, quantity and density of drinking, but not for passouts. Kaprio's group found heritability rates that were similar to those of Partanen: .39 for frequency of beer consumption, .38 for frequency of spirit consumption, .40 for density of consumption and .36 for quantity of consumption. Partanen's work was also confirmed by Gabrielli and Plomin (1985) whose sample included 46 M2 and 44 DZ twin pairs as well as 46 unrelated pairs of subjects who were raised together. 203 subjects were females and 143 were males; median level of schooling was 14.5 years. Unrelated pairs of adoptees were included to assess the importance of 7 shared family environment in alcohol consumption, independent of genetic influence. The Colorado Alcohol Behavior Questionnaire was used to assess amount, frequency and rate of alcohol consumption. Gabrielli and Plomin found significantly higher correlations of both amount and rate of alcohol consumption among MZ twins. Based on this data, they calculated a 66% heritability for rate and 25% heritability for amount of alcohol consumption. Gabrielli and Plomin concluded that genetic influences on drinking behavior were more important than shared family environment. The classic twin study which investigated alcohol abuse among twin pairs was Kaij's (1960). Kaij's subjects were 292 Swedish twins. At least one member of each twin pair had been registered with Temperence Boards set up to control excessive drinking. After categorizing each twin in terms of alcohol consumption, Kaij found that the co—twin of an index MZ twin was much more likely to fall into the same category of alcohol consumption as his twin than the co-twin of an index DZ twin. For example, he found that among chronic alcoholics, 71.4% of co-twins in M2 pairs fell in the same category as their index twin, while this was only true of 32.3% of co-twins in DZ pairs. Based on these results, Kaij concluded that alcohol abuse was largely an inherited trait. Murray, Clifford and Gurling (1983), however, criticized Kaij's findings on several grounds. One criticism was the low proportion of M2 twins in the study. This raised the possibility that some MZ twin pairs were mistakenly labelled as DZ. How this would lower the drinking concordance rate in the D2 group relative to the M2 group was not explained, however. A more important criticism presented by Murray's group was that Kaij's sample was not representative of alcoholics in general. For example, alcoholics registered with the Temperence Board were much more likely to have been convicted of alcohol-related criminal acts than most alcoholics. A more recent study of twins with drinking problems was conducted by Hrubec and Omenn (1981). Hrubec and Omenn accessed records of the VA to collect data on alcohol- 8 dependent twin pairs. They found MZ twins to be more concordant than DZ twins on rates of alcoholism, cirrhosis of the liver, and alcohol psychosis. Murray et a1. (1983) also criticized Hrubec and Omenn's methodology. They pointed out that overall detection rate of alcoholics among veterans in the study was lower than would be expected. They argued that the higher concordance rate among MZ twins than among DZ twins could be explained away if alcoholic cootwins of M2 index alcoholics were simply identified more often than alcoholic co-twins of DZ index alcoholics. Murray and his coworkers felt that alcoholic co-twins of M2 probands were indeed more likely to be identified by the armed forces because their relatedness was more noticeable than that of DZ twins. It has been argued that twin studies which find higher concordance rates for M2 than DZ twins do so simply because MZ twins share a more similar environment than DZ twins (Scarr, 1968). For example, parents may be more likely to treat their children alike if the children look identical then they may be to treat their children alike if the children do n0t resemble one another. Such factors could lead to more concordant drinking behavior among MZ than among DZ twins in the absence of genetic loading for alcoholism. However, several studies have disputed this argument by examining twins whose zygosity was incorrectly classified (Scarr and Carter-Saltzmann, 1970; Matheny, 1979). Results showed that true zygosity, not self-perceived zygosity or zygosity perceived by parents, determined similarity of the twins' behavior. Loehlin and Nichols' (1976) research also helped to resolve this issue. They correlated the similarity of twin pairs‘ social environment in childhood with the similarity of their scores on the California Personality Inventory and National Merit Test in adolescence. Results showed little or no correlation between similarity of social environment as children and similarity of intellectual and personality styles in high school. Therefore, it appears that the higher concordance rates of drinking behavior for M2 twins are not an artifact. They cannot be explained away by assuming that MZ twins share 9 a more similar environment than DZ twins. However, the concordance rates for M2 twins are not even close to the 100% one would expect if alcoholism was caused entirely by genetic factors. In order to differentiate the effects of nature and nurture more clearly, it is necessary to turn to adoption studies. 5 I . S 1' Adoption studies allow researchers to more clearly separate the effects of heredity from the effects of environment. Assuming that the child of an alcoholic biological parent is separated from that parent shonly after birth, it is possible to see if the child's pr0posed genetic predisposition for alcoholism affects him even in a family environment where. alcohol abuse does not occur. One of the earliest adoption studies on children of alcohol abusers was conducted by Roe and Burks (1945). Roe and her associate found that non; of the adopted-away sample of children of alcoholics and none of their matched controls showed any symptoms of alcohol dependence. They concluded that heredity had no influence on the development of alcoholism. However, these results may have been biased by the fact that children of alcoholic biological parents were more often placed in rural homes (where the opportunity to drink was infrequent) than children of non-alcoholic parentage (Cloninger et al., 1981). Also, because Roe and Burks never personally interviewed the natural parents, questions have arisen as to whether fathers of the index cases were really alcoholic (Goodwin et al., 1973). Finally, Schuckit (1980) has questioned Roe and Burks' results on the basis of small sample size. The adoption study which began the modern debate over the contribution of genetics to alcoholism was that of Goodwin and his colleagues in 1974 (Murray et al., 1983). Goodwin et al.'s (1974) sample consisted of male Danish adoptees. The index group was made up of sons of alcoholics raised by non-alcoholic foster parents while the control group was made up of sons of non-alcoholics paired on age and circumstances of ad0ption. 10 Data on sons of alcoholics who were raised by their biological parents was also included. Results showed that sons of alcoholic biological parents were four times more likely to become alcohol abusers than sons of non-alcoholic biological parents, regardless of whether or nor an alcoholic parent raised them. However, sons of alcoholics did not differ from controls when the dependent measure was rate of heavy drinking. These results strongly indicated that no matter what family environment sons of alcoholics were raised in, some type of genetic factor made them more vulnerable to alcoholism (but not to heavy drinking). Murray et a1. (1983) thought it curious that widening criteria for drinking pathology to include heavy drinking totally eliminated Goodwin's argument for a genetic diathesis in alcoholism. Pointing out that this contradicts the evidence that heavy drinking and alcoholism are closely related, they asked: " Could it be that Goodwin's findings are simply an artifact produced by the tlueshold for alcoholism accidentally di- viding heavy drinkers in the index and control group unevenly?" In another criticism of Goodwin's work, Fillmore (in press) suggested that Goodwin and colleagues' results were biased because of socioeconomic (SES) differences between the control and index group. She proposed that children of alcoholic biological parents would be more likely to be adopted out to lower class parents than children of normal biological parents. There is substantial evidence for the existence of social class differences in rates of alcoholism (Zucker, 1987a); (Cahalan and Cisin, 1976); more environmental press for deviance (i.e. alcoholism and antisocial behavior) seems to exist in lower socioeconomic groups. Given this fact, if adopted by lower SES parents, the index group in Goodwin's study would have been at higher risk for alcoholism simply because rates of 11 alcoholism are higher in lower class families. There would be no need to include heredity in an explanation of group differences. A different adoption study by Cadoret and Gath (1978) involved 84 adoptive families. Families were recruited through the 1939-1965 adoption records of the Iowa Childrens' and Family Services Agency. In six cases, the biological parents of the adoptive child were judged to have been alcoholic. A significant difference was found in alcoholism rates in the two groups such that adoptive children with an alcoholic biological parent were much more likely to become alcohol dependent. Questions about the validity of this study have been raised because of the use of parental interview to diagnose alcoholism in adoptees, the differential sample loss among families with alcoholic versus non-alcoholic adoptees, the possibility of impressionistic diagnosis of alcoholism in biological parents by adOption agency staff, and small sample size (Murray et al., 1983); (Fillmore, in press). However, a follow-up study by Cadoret, Troughton and O'Gorman (1985) was much more tightly controlled. The sample was drawn from Lutheran Social Services records. Alcoholism in the adoptees was diagnosed using DSM-III criteria during personal interviews. To assess sample loss, adoptive families who refused to participate were evaluated for alcohol use and found to be equivalent to participating families. The same type of results were found as before, indicating a genetic influence on the development of alcohol abuse for both males and females. Some of the best data from adoption studies comes from the research of C. Robert Cloninger and Martin Bohman. One of their studies (Cloninger, Bohman and Sigvardsson, 1981) involved 862 male Swedish adoptees, of whom 151 had some record of alcohol abuse. Subjects were part of the Stockholm Adoption Study. The sample included all persons born out of wedlock between 1930 and 1949 who were placed for adoption. Each of the adoptees, based on his drinking behavior, was classified as belonging to one of four groups. Data about both adoptive and natural parents was collected as well. Analysis of Cloninger et al.'s data showed a significant correlation 12 between alcohol dependence in the biological parents and alcohol dependence in the adoptees. The researchers also wished to know if the biological parents of severely alcohol dependent adoptees had differed from biological parents of mildly alcohol dependent adoptees. They were able to demonstrate the existence of two types of alcoholism in the alcohol abusing adoptees which were associated with psychological standing of the natural parents. One type of alcoholism (male-limited or Type II) was proposed to be highly heritable from father to son and to result in a moderate degree of alcoholism in the son. Supporting this hypothesis, male-limited alcoholics were found to have no excess of alcoholic mothers. In addition, Type II alcoholism was found to be associated with criminality and severe alcoholism in the adoptees' natural father. Therefore, in the male- limited alcoholic, antisocial behavior and alcoholism were found to be closely linked. Cloninger, Sigvardsson and Bohman (1988) also found that personality traits associated with antisocial behavior such as novelty-seeking behavior and harm avoidance which were measured in a large sample of eleven year olds predicted early-onset alcoholism (i.e. Type 2) in adulthood. Such information is important in light of studies by Vaillant (1983) and Mchd and McCord (1962) showing that childhood antisocial behavior is a good predictor of later alcohol problems. The Other type of alcoholism described by Cloninger et al. (milieu-limited or Type I) was proposed to be somewhat heritable from either biological parent and was associated with milder alcohol dependence and no record of criminality in the natural parents. In addition, Type I alcoholism was thought to be influenced by post-natal environmental factors, and to result in either a mild or severe degree of alcohol abuse depending on the degree of post-natal environmental stress. In order to reconcile these findings with Goodwin's (1974), which implied that family environment was not important in the development of alcoholism, Cloninger et al. (1985) suggested that Goodwin's sample consisted of male-limited, not milieu-limited, alcoholics. 1 3 A replication of the group's results among 913 female Swedish adoptees from the Stockholm Adoption Study (Bohman et al., 1981) confirmed the initial findings. First, there was a three-fold excess of alcohol abusers among adopted daughters of alcoholic biological mothers as compared to daughters of non-alcoholics. Biological fathers with a record of criminality and severe alcoholism had very few alcoholic daughters. This is consistent with the prediction that male-limited alcohlism is mostly passed on to sons. However, there was a high degree of alcohol abuse among. daughters of biological parents who were not involved in criminal activity and whose alcohol abuse was mild, supporting the idea that milieu-limited alcoholism is heritable by either sex. Bohman et al. concluded that alcoholism in women generally fit the Type 1 pattern. The work of Bohman and Cloninger is important for several reasons. It not only replicated earlier findings of a genetic contribution to alcoholism, but it was one of the firsr pieces of literature to suggest different degrees of heritability for different types of alcoholism. Considered from this perspective, it is possible that adoption studies such as Roe and Burks' (1945) found no evidence for a genetic contribution to alcoholism simply because they studied children of 'milieu limited' alcoholics in adaptive family environments (Cloninger et al., 1985). As stated earlier, the validity of this research can be questioned because of its reliance on registration with the Temperence Board to define alcoholism. The fact that legal criteria for registration with the Temperence Board changed over time calls into question the similarity of parental alcoholics to their alcoholic offspring (Fillmore, in press). At the very least, it may limit how generalizable Bohman and Cloninger's results are to other alcoholic populations. Searles (1988) has also pointed out several flaws in the Cloninger studies. He nored that in contrast to data from epidemiological studies, Cloninger and his colleagues found no increased risk for alcohol abuse as a funCtion of age in their sample. This could be another indication that their sample is atypical of alcoholics in general. In addition, Searles pointed 14 out that the group's system for classifying subjects as mild, moderate or severe was inadequate; he suggested that their findings might simply be an artifact of their criteria for abuse. The evidence from b0th adoption and twin studies leads to similar conclusions. Confirming the findings from twin studies which indicated that genetic factors influenced the development of alcoholism, most of the adoption studies found higher rates of alcoholism among adoptees whose natural parents had been alcoholics. Although some of the studies were methodologically flawed, the fact that replications kept producing the same pattern of results indicates that there is indeed a genetic component to the development of alcohol abuse. However, as a caveat, it is important to remember that it is not at all clear that parents who give their children up for adoption are representative of the general alcoholic population; they may show more signs of antisocial behavior (Murray et al., 1983). Lastly, there was little consensus among adoption studies about the degree to which pOSt-natal family environment affects genetic loading for alcoholism. There is obviously a need for well-controlled studies which define alcoholism in a useful way and which also have large samples such as Cloninger et al.'s (1981). Also, there is a need for research which makes even greater disrinctions between alcoholic subpopulations and the degree of heritability of each. If one assumes that alcoholism is indeed influenced by genetic factors, then what exactly is being inherited? Factors from biochemical abnormalities to temperament differences have been implicated. The evidence for such factors will now be reviewed. 1M 25111.11”, Schuckit (1980), in a broad overview of possible biological mediators of alcoholism, suggested five possible mechanisms through which a genetic predisposition for alcoholism could express itself. The first of these was that individuals at risk for alcoholism could inherit different acute responses to doses of alcohol. For example, alcohol might produce 15 more intense pleasure for alcoholics. The second of these was that high risk individuals might inherit different subacute reactions to alcohol. As a third mechanism through which a genetic predisposition could express itself, Schuckit proposed that high-risk individuals might be more vulnerable to chronic alcohol exposure than other individuals. A fourth proposed mechanism was differences in the way individuals at risk metabolize alcohol as compared to individuals not at risk. Finally, Schuckit suggested that high-risk individuals might inherit factors which affected psychological parameters such as temperament. 3.] .1” 1.. It is often hypothesized that alcoholics inherit a biochemical abnormality which somehow affects their interactions with alcohol. Schuckit and Rayses (1979) proposed that alcoholics produce higher amounts of acetaldehyde than non-alcoholics; because alcetaldehyde is a breakdown product of alcohol metabolism in the liver, genetic variations in the efficiency of alcohol-metabolizing enzymes would affect acetaldehyde concentrations in the body. To prove that this was an inherited vulnerability, Schuckit and Rayses compared non-alcoholic subjects with a positive family history of alcoholism to matched controls with no family history of alcoholism. Results confirmed that after drinking alcohol, the subjects with a positive family history had significantly higher breath concentrations of acetaldehyde. Unfortunately, attempts to replicate this important finding have been unsuccessful (Knop, Angelo and Christensen,1981). Another biochemical abnormality which may be inherited by alcoholics is low levels of monoamine oxidase (MAO), an mitochondrial enzyme that catalyzes the oxidative deamination of biogenic amines. (Faraj, Lenton, Kutner, Camp, Stammers, Lee, Lolies and Chandora, 1987). Monoamine oxidase is involved in brain neurotransmitter metabolism, but is also found in blood platelets. (Alcohol and Health, 1987). Initial research suggested that chronic alcoholics had MAO levels which were lower than normal (Oreland et al., 1983); (Faraj et al., 1987). Puchall, Coursey, Buchsbaum and Murphy 1 6 (1983) were than able to demonstrate that MAO level was genetically determined. 75 subjects with either high or low MAO levels were chosen and MAO level was correlated with that of their parents. Results showed significant and positive correlations. Low MAO levels have been shown to be correlated with a tendency to increase or 'augment' stimulus intensity (Buchsbaum, Landau, Murphy and Goodwin, 1973); alcoholics as a group are likely to be stimulus augmenters (Petrie, 1967). In addition, low MAO levels are associated with the type of fast tempo and vigorous behavioral response style which is typical of alcoholics (Tarter, Alterrnan and Edwards 1985). Von Knorring, Bohman, Von Knorring and Oreland (1985), building upon the work of Cloninger and Bohman, first showed that the male- limited (highly heritable)/ milieu- limited (somewhat heritable) typology could validly differentiate alcoholics in a clinical setting. The typology was used to classify 31 male and five female alcoholics treated through a university outpatient psychiatric clinic. They then demonstrated that the MAO levels of milieu-limited alcoholics did not differ significantly from those of healthy controls, whereas male-limited alcoholics had significantly lower MAO levels than controls. Findings of lower MAO levels among Type 2 alcoholics were confirmed by Pandey, Fawcett, Gibbons, Clark and Davis (1988). E . El 1.. Some researchers feel that alcoholics inherit an anomalous brain structure which leads to some type of neurological dysfunction. Schuckit (1984) proposed that alcoholics were less able than non-alcoholics to use internal cues to estimate their blood alcohol level (BAL) after drinking. His sample consisted of 23 non-alcoholic male college students with either a positive or negative family history of alcoholism. After consuming alcohol, subjects with a family history of alcoholism had significantly lower self-ratings of intoxication than controls. These results indicate that alcoholics may inherit a deficit in the ability to learn to 1 7 process cues about internal state, especially when the internal state experienced is alcohol- induced. Several studies have been conducted on the electroencephalograms (131305) of persons at high risk for alcoholism. Propping (1977), in a study of 52 healthy twin pairs, showed that the extent of alcohol action on the resting EEG was under genetic control. After giving subjects a dose of ethanol, he recorded their EEG's; EEG's of M2 twins reacted identically to alcohol loading whereas EBG's of DZ twins became more dissimilar. Propping and his colleagues then conducted a follow-up study on relatives of alcoholics and matched controls (Propping, Kruger and Nark, 1981). They found that non-drinking females with a positive family history of alcoholism had a significantly poorer EEG synchronization than female controls. No such effect was found for males, however. Pollock, Volavka, Mednick, Goodwin et al. (1984) found that after consuming alcohol, 44 subjects at high risk for alcoholism could be differentiated from 28 matched controls by their EEG alpha frequencies. Subjects in the high-risk group showed significantly greater increases in slow alpha frequencies and decreases in fast alpha frequencies. The researchers interpreted the results to mean that EEGs could function as a biological marker for an inherited central nervous system (CNS) sensitivity to the effects of alcohol among alcoholics. Gabrielli, Mednick, Volavka, Pollock et al. (1982) found that 27 young high- risk children of alcoholics showed more beta wave activity in their EEG's than 27 matched controls. Begleiter, Porjesz, Bihari and Kissin (1984) studied visually produced event-related brain pctentials (ERPs) among 25 non-drinking sons of alcoholic fathers and matched controls with no family history of alcoholism. They found significant group differences in the P3 component of the ERP. Begleiter's group proposed that because P3 potentials reflect processes involved in revising representations stored in memory, alcoholics might inherit deficits in memory processing. 18 While this area of research is promising, a major problem exists. Although each study on inherited brain wave abnormalities has found subjects at high risk for alcoholism to differ from healthy controls, two studies have rarely found the same anomalous brain wave patterns (Peele,1986). Therefore, findings may well be sample specific. Also, none of these studies have discussed in depth how an inherited brain abnormality would lead to alcoholism. War It is also possible that alcoholics inherit a temperament that makes them more vulnerable to alcohol abuse. Tarter, Alterman and Edwards (1985), in an elegant discussion, identified six temperament dimensions which might play a part in vulnerability to alcoholism. These six factors were activity level, attention span /persistence, soothability, emotionality, reaction to food and sociability. Tarter's group presented evidence that at least some of these temperament dimensions had a genetic component and then discussed possible underlying biological mechanisms. This piece of work is important because it is one of the few papers which describes how inherited biological dysfunctions could express themselves as factors which would hinder an alcoholic's functioning in his environment. Sratemenmflhefimblem After reviewing the literature, it seems clear that some genetically-based vulnerability to alcoholism exists. Animal models have been developed which show that true-breeding, alcohol-preferrin g strains of rats can be produced with relative ease. The evidence from twin and adoption studies also indicates that the children of alcoholic parents inherit some factor which places them at risk for alcoholism. However, much of the research suffers 19 from poor methodology and problems with the way in which alcoholism is defined. Therefore, generalizability becomes questionable. Moreover, as previously discussed, although the best evidence which supports the theory of genetic loading for alcoholism comes from adoption studies, it is not at all clear that parents who give their children up for adoption are representative of the general alcoholic population. If adoption studies have indeed used samples which tend to be more heavily involved in antisocial behavior, it is possible that alcoholism is highly heritable only among such groups. The present study addresses this issue by studying genetic loading for alcoholism within a sample who were not adopted out by their natural parents. Furthermore, few studies have investigated how genetic loading for alcoholism might influence various different drinking-related variables; those which have done so have only considered a limited range of factors, such as frequency and amount of drinking. This study, using male subjects, examines the relationship between genetic loading for alcoholism (as measured by family expression of alcoholism) and age of first drunkenness, lifetime number of areas of drinking problems, percent of the lifespan characterized by alcohol problems and quantity-variability and frequency of drinking. In addition, alcoholic male subjects are characterized as Type I or Type II alcoholics in order to test whether these different drinking-related variables are more heritable among male-limited alcoholics. Such data are unique in that they provide information on the degree to which familial (and ostensibly genetic) density of alcoholism affects the onset, duration and severity of alcohol problems. They also help differentiate which aspects of drinking problems are under genetic control. Lastly, the study examines the role of other posited factors that moderate heritability of alcohol abuse. While the effects of factors such as environmental press for deviance (as defined by SES), exposure to an alcoholic caretaker and antisocial behavior have been touched upon in various studies of genetic loading for alcoholism, they have been addressed neither simultaneously nor systematically. Due to the nature of the sample, 20 findings from this study are only generalizable to a more impulsive population in which drinking problems are already substantial. Hypotheses Hypothesis 1a: There will be a positive correlation between genetic loading for alcoholism and problems with alcohol in adulthood (i.e. early onset of drinking-related difficulties, longer percentage of the lifespan characterized by alcohol problems, high number of lifetime problems with alcohol and heavy intake of alcohol as defined by quantity, frequency and variability of alcohol use). Hypothesis 1b: Genetic loading for alcoholism will only be positively correlated with the variables listed in Hyporhesis 1a among Type 2 alcoholics. Hypothesis 2: Current environmental press for deviance (as defined by adult SES) will moderate the effects of genetic loading for alcoholism. Higher SES will be related to fewer alcohol problems in respondents. Hypothesis 3: Environmental press for deviance during childhood (as defined by childhood SES) will moderate the effects of genetic loading for alcoholism. Respondents with higher SES during childhood will have fewer alcohol problems in adulthood than will respondents with lower SES during childhood. Hypothesis 4: Exposure to an alcoholic environment as a child (e.g. being reared by an alcoholic) will moderate the effects of genetic loading for alcoholism. Respondents who were reared by alcoholic parents will have more alcohol-related difficulties than those who WCTC DOt. Hypothesis 5: Childhood antisocial behavior will moderate the effect of genetic loading for alcoholism. Respondents who displayed childhood antisocial behavior will have more alcohol problems than those who did nor. Hypothesis 6a: Adult antisocial behavior will be highly correlated with alcoholism. 21 2 2 Hyp0thesis 6b: Adult antisocial behavior will be highly correlated with alcoholism among Type 2 but not among Type 1 alcoholics. Methods Meets This study utilized data from 125 men participating in the Michigan State University Longitudinal Study (Zucker, Noll and Fitzgerald, 1986). The larger research project uacks the deveIOpment of a group of families with children who are at elevated risk for conducr disorders and alcoholism because of their male gender and because their fathers are alcoholic. These alcoholic men are identified from the population of all males convicted of drunk driving in the mid-Michigan Tri-County area. In order to meet selection criteria, potential respondents must have had a blood alcohol concentration (BAL) of. 15% (150 mg/ 100ml) or higher when arrested (indicating the development of substantial alcohol tolerance) or must have had a BAL of .12% but also have had a history of an additional alcohol-related driving offense. In addition, potential respondents must have a male offspring between three and six years of age and must be residing with the child's mother at initial contact. After being approached by court probation officers about the study, those men who agree to be contacted (currently 77%) are recruited into the M.S.U. Longitudinal Study by project staff (the acceptance rate at this stage is 90%). Subjects are screened using items from the SMAST (Short Michigan Alcohol Screening Test; Selzer, 1975) shortly after recruitment and again later with items from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS Version III) (Robins, Helzer, Croughan and Ratcliffe, 1980) to verify that they do indeed meet the Feighner research diagnostic criteria (Feighner, Robins, Winokur, Guze et. al., 1972) for either probable or definite alcoholism. Currently, 88% of fathers in the study meet a definite diagnosis. Participating families are compensated at the completion of each wave of data collection; families currently receive $250 for their involvement. For the pruposes of this study, it is desirable to have a high degree of variance in the distribution of subjects' current level of alcohol abuse. Therefore, a subset of men has been included who come from families which function as matched controls for the alcoholic 23 24 Table 1 m hi h ° ‘ f X ( s.d.) Range Age 30.6 (4.6) 22-47 Years of Education 12.7 (4.0) 7 -20 Number of Marriages 1.2 (0.5) 0 -3 Annual Family Income $26,983 (16,294) $2,000-$62,500 Number of Children in 2.4 (0.9) 1-5 Household 25 These families reside in the same census tract as high risk families and are homogeneous with them for age of target male offspring, family economic status and neighborhood; however, neither parent may meet Feighner criteria for alcoholism or for other drug abuse/ dependence (see Table 1 for sample demographics). mm Data are collected by trained project staff who are blind to family risk status. Because of the large volume of data collected, a number of contacts with the family are necessary, involving 18 hours of time for each parent and seven hours of time for the target child. These contacts include quesrionnaire sessions, semi-strucrured interviews and interactive tasks. Six of the data collection sessions take place in the family home, while two take place on the M.S.U. campus. Genograms are collected from the family during the last contact of the first assessment phase. Subjects are first asked to produce a family tree which extends back to the grandparental generation and which includes such second degree relatives as aunts, uncles and first cousins. First names, sex, ages and/or birthdates of each relative are recorded if known by the subject. Subjects are then given a list of various physical and psychological disorders (Table 2) and, for each disorder listed, asked if any of the persons recorded on the family tree suffered from it. Any additional information provided by subjects about their family, such as disorders not included on the standardized list, is also recorded. mm In order to determine whether alcoholic subjects fit a Type I or Type II classification, - an algorithm (Figure l) was used which utilizes the criteria laid out by Cloninger's group (Von Knorring et al., 1985; Cloninger, 1987). The measure uses questions from the 26 Tab1e2 E .1] E .1 II] a Alcoholism Allergies or Asthma Anemia Arthritis (Rheumatoid Arthritis) Cancer/ Leukemia Color Blindness Depression Diabetes Drug Abuse Emphysema Epilepsy/ Convulsion Disorder Gout Heart Problems/ Heart Disease High Blood Pressure (Hypertension) Hyperactivity Kidney/ Bladder Problems Learning Disabilities Liver Disease Manic Depressive Illness (Bipolar Disorder) Nervous Breakdown Schizophrenia Stroke Suicide Tuberculosis Tumor Other Illness a List given to respondents during the genogram interview 2 7 Meets DSM-III Criteria No _ For Alcohol Dependence Nonalcohohc \LYes Three of the following Social Complications (or two if DIS aQ #164 endorsed): 1. Arguments with family about drinking DIS # 150 2. Absent from work due to drinking DIS #160 3. Loss of job due to drinking DIS #161 4. Legal Difficulties due to drinking DIS #162, 163 5. Violent while intoxicated DIS #164 \ No , Alcohol Problems Began Before Age 25 One of the Following Psychological Complications: \l/NO 1. Thought I was drinking too much No DDHb #2 . 2. Felt guilty about drinking > I Indeterminate DDH #6 Yes Yes Type I or Milieu-Limited l Type II or Male-Limi ted * Figure 1 D' fiQ'fi' [€111.51 a b DIS #3 refer to questions from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule-III DDH #5 refer to items from Drinking and Drug History 2 8 D18- Version III (Robins et al., 1980) and the Drinking and Drug History (Zucker and N011, 1980) to determine alcoholic subtype. Both these instruments provide information about problems associated with alcohol use. Subjecrs who met Feighner criteria for probable or definite alcoholism, whose drinking had not incurred social consequences (such as loss of a job) and who had suffered psychological distress over their drinking were coded as Type I alcoholics. Subjects who met Feighner criteria for probable or definite alcoholism, whose drinking began before age 25 and whose drinking had incurred social complications were coded as Type II alcoholics. £12.]. 111]. ”112” 5 1,25: In order to determine degree of alcohol related difficulty over the lifecourse, the LAPS (Zucker, 1989) was used. Information from which LAPS is determined is provided by the DIS, Drinking and Drug History and SMAST. LAPS is a composite score derived from three different components: age of first drunkeness, variety of alcohol problems and life percent involving alcohol problems These three subscores are standardized and are calculated as 1) the squared reciprocal of age at which respondent first reported being drunk 2) the number of areas in which drinking problems are reported and 3) the number of years between respondents' first drinking problem and most recent drinking problem multiplied by the squared reciprocal of his age. The measure effecrively distinguishes between _ alcoholics and non- alcoholics and is moderately to strongly correlated with a range of external measures of alcohol-related difficulty. WW To determine quantity, frequency and variability of drinking, data from the Drinking and Drug History (Zucker and N011, 1980) was utilized. This self-report 29 questionnaire assesses amount of alcohol intake in the past six months, using an extended version of the standardized survey questions developed by Cahalan, Cisin and Crossley (1969). Data are coded for quantity-variability and frequency of alcohol consumption, where frequency is a yearly figure based upon number of drinking episodes in the past six months (doubled) and quantity-variability taps both the modal amount of alcohol consumed on any given drinking occasion and the maximum amount consumed when there is variation in consumption. The measure used is a revised version of Cahalan, Cisin and Crossley's Alcohol Consumption Index: the QFV-R (Zucker and Davies, 1989), which provides a more extended range for the measure. Alcoholics who are not currently drinking are not included in computations that involve either of the current consumption measures. CW The measure used to obtain information on alcoholism in the families of subjects is the genogram. The genogram utilizes an interview method known as the family history method, where subjects provide data on psychiatric and physical disorders in other family members. (Thompson, Orvaschel, Prusoff and Kidd, 1982). Several studies have investigated the reliability and validity of the family history method. O'Malley, Carey and Maisto (1986) compared young adults' reports of alcohol use and alcohol-related problems in their parents to the parents' self-report; the two were found to be highly correlated (e. g. the Pearson correlation between students' and fathers' estimates of average monthly consumption was .72). Thompson et al. (1982) compared subjects' reports of various psychiatric illnesses in their relatives to diagnoses made by psychiatrists during personal interviews. They found that the family history method generated few false positives (specificity = .96) for alcoholism, but that subjects often classified alcoholic relatives as unaffected, producing many false negatives (sensitivity = .57). Offspring were found to produce the most accurate reports of illness, as compared to spouses and parents. 3 0 Thompson et al. concluded that positive diagnoses generated by the family history method are highly likely to be accurate, but that the true incidence of alcoholism in subjects' relatives will be underestimated. 1 Luff..- '101' in o no ' n O; ‘ :. ‘.~ 0 --t- -' harm The FEA score is not a true measure of genetic loading for alcoholism, as it includes data from respondents' siblings (who contribute no genetic material to the respondent). Rather, FEA indexes respondents' genetic loading for alcoholism by reflecting the density of alcoholism in respondents' families as well as the degree of relatedness of these alcoholic family members to respondents. In order to assign each subject a FEA score, alcoholic family members were identified by using the subjects' genograms. Although genograms provided data on cousins, only data about parents, siblings, grandparents, aunts and uncles were used in the analyses, since it became clear that in this data set, subjects' familiarity with more distant relatives was insufficient to allow them to accurately label these relatives as alcoholic or n0t. Next, the degree of relatedness between the subject and each alcoholic relative was determined. The degree of relatedness between two family members can be expressed by a value known in human genetics as the coefficient of relationship (Figure 2). Once coefficients of relationship were detemrined for the subject and each alcoholic relative, genetic loading scores were calculated by 1) within each generation, summing the coefficients of relationship for all alcoholic relatives 2) multiplying this sum by the ratio of alcoholics in each generation to the total number of family members in that generation and 3) summing the subscores across generations. A sample calculation of FEA is shown in Figure 3. 25"111' The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS-Version III) ( Robins et al., 1980) was used to measure childhood and adult antisocial behavior. The DIS is a semi- 31 O I O Grandparent (.25) I ' Grandparent Great Uncle/ Aunt O l (.25) (.125) Unc e/ Aunt Parent Parent FirSt Cousin (.25) (.50) (.50) Once Removed (.0625) Subject Sibling (.50) O = Common Ancestor O = Relative by Marriage Figure 2 - .. O- . ' ' ' ‘ 0 . o .A. . x '. . I...) I‘k‘.’ '_ '. . 1.1.! a..'".' t." 32 Grandfather Grandmother Grandfather Alcoholism Asthma/ Allergies Heart Disease Grandmmhe" Alzheimer's .25 .25 .50 .50 I Mother 6 213111? lism Alcoholism co 0 Aunt Uncle Uncle - Aunt Aunt Uncle Kidney Problems 5 l a J) 1.50 J] 4) Sister Br0ther rother Sister Alcoholism I = alcoholic Subject Step 1: within each generation, sum the coefficients of relationship for all alcoholic relatives 61: .25 + 0 = .25 G2: .25 + .50 + .50 = 1.25 G3: .50 + 0 = .50 Step 2: multiply this sum by the ratio of alcoholics in each generation to the toral number of family members in that generation 61: (.25).25 = .06 G2: (1.25).38 = .48 G3: (.50) .20 = .10 Step 3: sum subscores across generations FEA = .06 + .48 + .10 =.64 Figure 3 33 structured interview developed for use in a multisite study of rates of mental disorder in the general population (Helzer, Robins, McEvoy, Spitnagel, Stolzmann, Farmer and Brockington, 1985). It was designed to make diagnoses by three systems: DSM—III, the Feighner criteria and Research Diagnostic Criteria (Robins, Helzer, Croughan and Ratcliff, 1980). Inter-rater reliability of the DIS has been reported to be high (k=.94) (Hesselbrook, Stabenau, Hesselbrook, Mirkin and Meyer, 1982), as has its test-retest reliability (Wittchen, Burke, Semler, Pfister, Cranach and Zandig, 1989). Validity of the DIS has been tested by examining its concordance with the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Lifetime (Endicott and Spitzer, 1978); agreement across diagnoses was found to be quite high (k=.80) (Hesselbrook et al., 1982). The DIS was used to obtain a count of the number of DSM-III symptoms of child and adult antisocial behavior experienced by each subject. E S . . S ESESI Information used to rate socioeconomic status comes from the Demographic Questionnaire (Zucker and Noll,1980). Childhood and adulthood SES of subjects was calculated using the Duncan TSE12 Socioeconomic Index (Stevens and Featherman, 1981), an occupationally-based measure of social prestige. Significant evidence exists in the sociological literature to suggest that occupation, not income or education, is the optimal indicator of SES and that the perceived prestige of an occupation best captures its underlying socioeconomic dimension (Mueller and Parcel, 1981). WWW To assess degree of exposure to an alcoholic environment (e. g. being reared by an alcoholic parent or stepparent) as a child, the Demographic Questionnaire and genograms were used. The Demographic Questionnaire identified people involved in 34 raising respondents, while genogram data provided a means for assessing drinking status of subjects' parents and/or other primary caretakers. Degree of exposure to an alcoholic environment was rated on a scale which assigned one point for each alcoholic involved in raising the respondent (e.g. parents, stepparents, grandparents). An additional point was added for any pairing of alcoholic caretakers (e.g. two alcoholic parents). Thus, a respondent raised by an alcoholic mother would receive a score of one, whereas a respondent raised by an alcoholic father and an alcoholic stepmother would receive a score of three. Additional points were added for a pairing because of the potentiating effect of having two caretakers who both agree about, rather than have conflict over, heavy consumption (Reider, Zucker, Maguin, N011 and Fitzgerald, 1989). Results Pearson product-moment correlations between family expression of alcoholism and alcohol involvement variables for the entire sample of men (including non-alcoholics) are presented in Table 3. Correlations were significant between FEA and LAPS (p.32, p<.001), age of first drunkeness (r,=-.28, p<.01), lifetime variety of alcohol problems (1:.33, p<.001) and frequency of alcohol consumption in the last six months (1:.26, p<.01). W Alcoholic males in the sample were further classified as either Type 1 or Type 2. Some of these men (14%) were not codable as either type; they were excluded from these analyses. Inspection of scatterplots of FEA and LAPS revealed distributions for Type ls and 25 which were similar to that of the overall sample of men; no significant skewness was found, justifying continued analyses of these two subtypes. The two types of alcoholics were found to differ on a number of variables which assess life difficulty. As shown in Table 4, t-tests demonstrated that Type 2 alcoholics had significantly lower socioeconomic status (1: 2.58, p<.01). They also reported significantly higher levels of child antisocial behavior (t=-5.86, p<.0001) and adult antisocial behavior (1=-6.43, p<.0001) than did Type 1 alcoholics. Moreover, they experienced significantly more separations/ divorces from their partners (t=-4.23, p<.0001). Type 2 alcoholics also scored significantly higher on Lifetime Alcohol Problems Score (t=-3.28, p < .002). However, the two groups did not differ significantly on consumpu'on variables such as frequency of drinking in the past six months or quantity-variability of drinking in the 3 5 36 Table 3 0111'v'. _3 ' l 1.; '1 _ o ' i o ‘ thi r -_ 0 .1 chlo_ VOW 0 Mn. MAW (n=124) LAPS a .32 * * Age First Drunk b --28 * Variety C .33 * * Life Percentd .07 WW (n=100) Quantity-Variability .16 Frequency .26 * 11919.. Only current drinkers are included for the correlations involving consumption indices. a Lifetime Alcohol Problems Score 9 Age of First Drunkeness C Lifetime Variety of Alcohol Problems ‘1 Life Percent Involving Alcohol Problems e Quantity-Variability of Alcohol Consumption fFrequency of Alcohol Consumption *p<.01**p<.001 or..- or L ”roar-.- ' X (s.d.) Typel Socioeconomic Status (Duncan TSE12) 35.5 (16.4) Number of Child Antisocial Behavior 1.7 ( 1.5) Symptoms from DIS Number of Adult Antisocial Behavior 2.5 ( 1.0) Symptoms from DIS Number of Separations/ Divorces from Partner(s) .9 (1.0) Lifetime Alcohol Problems Score 9.8 ( 1.4) QFV-R Frequency Classification 6.2 ( 2.6) QFV-R Quantity- Variability Classification 13.4 ( 6.7) Beck Depression Inventory Score 2.6 ( 2.9) Type2 26.2 (12.0) 4.4 (2.8) 4.4 (1.7) 2.2 (1.8) 11.2 ( 2.0) 5.9 (2.8) 15.0 ( 6.9) 3.8 ( 3.7) 2.58 * -5.86 * * * -6.43 * * * -4.23 * * * -3.28 * * .54 -.84 -1.45 rpb .50 .69 .71 .61 .55 .26 .32 .38 Note, Figures in parentheses are standard deviations. *p<.05 **p<.01***p<.001 38 past six months. Current level of depression, as indexed by scores on the short form of the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck and Beck, 1972) also did not differ between the two groups. The relationship between alcohol involvement variables and family expression of alcoholism for Type 1 and Type 2 alcoholics is presented in Table 5. For Type 25, family expression of alcoholism was found to be significantly positively correlated to overall LAPS (1:.27, p<.05) and to the LAPS subscore assessing lifetime variety of alcohol problems (p.30, p< .05). Frequency of alcohol consumption was also significantly related to FEA (r:=.38, p _<_ .01). For Type I alcoholics, all correlations between FEA and drinking variables were found to be non-significant. However, sample size in the Type 1 group is small; moreover, the magnitude of the relationships between FEA and drinking variables was not significantly different in the Type 2 group than in the Type 1 group. Therefore, initial analyses do not support the hypothesis that drinking difficulties are more heritable among Type 2 alcoholics. Because the FEA measure of genetic loading includes the contribution of alcoholic parents, it is confounded with the environmental effects of being reared by an alcoholic caretaker. In order to estimate the relationship between FEA and alcohol involvement variables while adjusting for the effects of being reared by an alcoholic caretaker, partial correlations were also calculated; alcoholic rearing environment was used as the control variable. Table 6 shows the correlations between FEA and the alcohol involvement variables after the effects of being reared by an alcoholic were statistically controlled. For Type 2 alcoholics, the correlation between family expression of alcoholism and LAPS remained significant (1:.28, p<.05), and virtually identical, as did the correlations between family expression of alcoholism and lifetime variety of alcohol problems (I: .28, p< .05) and between FEA and frequency of alcohol consumption (r: .36, p< .05). FEA and age of first drunkeness were also found to be significantly negatively correlated ( r = -.27, 39 Table 5 0111’ ,9” :* gfi. ,3. Jo- 'o e A 001i gm. 10101VO’1V n 701' 0“ WW Type 1 Type 2 WM (n=25) (n=60) LAPS a .18 .27 * Age First Drunk b --33 --23 Variety c .27 .30 * we Pcrcentd ’.18 '20] WW (n=20) (n=42) Quantity-Variability -.14 .26 Frequency .03 .38 * * 151912, Only current drinkers are included for the correlations involving consumption indices. a Lifetime Alcohol Problems Score b Age of First Drunkeness 9 Lifetime Variety of Alcohol Problems ‘1 Life Percent Involving Alcohol Problems e Quantity-Variability of Alcohol Consumption fFrequency of Alcohol Consumption *p<.05 **ps.01 4O Table 6 -02 ‘ltru =' "v.1! 1).. .u‘ or o a. on n 1.9. a- no ,rVVO'n'r 0 .40; V T»; ‘ ochli L r onus in r '41 1A1 0011115 Ll ’ ‘- 'n Enzircnment Type 1 Type 2 1 1 1 1 ((1:25) (n=60) LAPS a -.21'*' .28 * + Age First Drunk b .13 -.27 * Variety C _ -.01 .28 * Life Percent d '35 -02 WWW (n=20) (n=42) Quantity-Variability -.35 .17 Frequency -.15 .36 * 1191;, Only current drinkers are included for the correlations involving consumption indices. a Lifetime Alcohol Problems Score 9 Age of First Drunkeness C Lifetime Variety of Alcohol Problems ‘1 Life Percent Involving Alcohol Problems 3 Quantity-Variability of Alcohol Consumption fFrequency of Alcohol Consumption * p < .05 + coefficients with this superscript differ significantly from each Other (p< .05) 41 p<.05). Therefore, these relationships are unaffected by rearing environment. In contrast, for Type 1 alcoholics correlations between FEA and drinking variables remained non- significant, but the direction of the relationship was now found to be Opposite from that originally Observed. In addition, a significant difference was now found in the magnitude Of the correlation between FEA and LAPS for Type 1 and Type 2 alcoholics (;= 2.00, p< .05). The fact that partialling out the shared variance between FEA and alcoholic rearing environment differentially affected the relationship between FEA and LAPS for Type 15 versus Type 25 is consistent with findings that a significant difference (7,: 2.14, p < .05) existed in the magnitude of the correlation between FEA and rearing environment for Type 15 (1:39, p < .01) and Type 25 (x=.74, p < .01). 31.1.] ”1.51112” 5 10! l'fiE'Efi 1. Correlations between life difficulty variables for Type 2 alcoholics are presented in Table 7 (figures below the diagonal). For these men, LAPS was found to be significantly positively correlated with childhood antisocial behavior (p.39, p< .01) and adult antisocial behavior (1: .41, p<.01) as well as with FEA (1:.27, p<.05). Childhood antisocial behavior was also positively correlated with adult antisocial behavior (p.31, p<.05). NO significant relationship between LAPS and socioeconomic status was found. However, childhood antisocial behavior was found to be negatively correlated with adult SES (3:: - .32, p<.05). Adult antisocial behavior was also negatively correlated with adult SES (1‘: - .36, p<.01). Table 7 also shows the relationships between life difficulty variables for Type 1 alcoholics (figures above the diagonal). Probably as a result Of low power, few correlations were significant. Patterns were also different from those found in the Type 2 group. For Type 1 men, LAPS was found to be significantly correlated only with being reared in an alcoholic environment $.42, p< .05); neither childhood nor adulthood antisocial 42 Table 7 our tun tutor: f hi! - a Vat-1' f0 , n; = t u 1—01. mm ' CASBa AASBb FEAC csr~:s LAPS .74 * * Alcoholic Rearing Environment .65 o it * * p < .01 r 2 = .20 .16 Childhood Antisocial Behavior 36\ \L -_01 .23+ Family Expression of Alcoholism > LAPS .39" * -.06 Alcoholic Rearing Environment +p<.10 **p<.01 Figure 6 Full Models of Digal Predictfi of LAPS for Tm 1 and Type; 2 Alcoholics Appendix B Demographic Questionnaire 58 OEHO ll/89 Respondent Number: Given By: Date: ngHlGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Tl.0 FAMILY ERQJECT Ans. Chk: East Lansing. Hichigan 48824 Background information We would like to ask you a few questions about yourself. The questions ask about your life during the time you were growing up as well as now. Please answer gll of them as completely as possible. (PLEASE PRINT). 1. What is your date of birth? MONTH DAY YEAR 2. Where were you born? CITY/TOWN (COUNTY 1F RURAL) STATE COUNTRY (IF NOT U.S.) 3. Where did you live most of the time until you were 18? CITY/TOWN (COUNTY lF RURAL) STATE COUNTRY (lF NOT U.S.) 4. Until you were 18. about how many times did your family move. CIRCLE ONE 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7 or more. 5a. Did you live together with both of your natural parents for most of the time from birth to 18? ClRCLE ONE. YES (if Yes. go to question 6) N0 (if NO. go to question 5b) so. What was the main reason your parents did not live together with you during that time? CIRCLE ONE 1. Hother died 2. Father died 3. Both parents died 4. Parents divorced or separated 5. Parents never lived together 6. Other (Please explain) :59 So. Which adultis) did you live with most of the time from birth to 18? CIRCLE ONE. 2. Mother. but no adult male 3. Father. but no adult female 4. Mother and step-father 5. Father and step-mother 6. Other (Please explain) 6. Who was the main wage earner in your home while you were growing up? CHECK ONE (a) your father (b) your mother (c) someone else What was their relationship to you ABOUT YOUR NATURAL (BIOLOGICAL) Fxrggg 7a. Where was he born? STATE COUNTRY (IF NOT U.S.) ABOUT THE ADULT HALE WHO LIVED WITH YOU NOST OF Tfl§7TIHE UNTIL YOU WERE 18. (This could be your natural father. or stepfather. or someone else). 7b. What kind of work did this adult male do (the adult male who lived with you most of the time until you were 18?) That is. what was his occupation? (For example: electrical engineer. machinist. stock clerk. assembly line worker. farmer) 7c. What were his most important activities or duties? (For example: keep account books. filing. selling cars. operate printing press. finish concrete) 7d. What kind of business or industry was this? (For exarrple: TV and radio mfg.. Retail shoe store. Automobile manufacturing [Oldsmobile]. State Labor Dept.. Farm work) 7e. What was the highest grade of school he completed? CIRCLE THE HIGHEST GRADE COHPLETED. None 0 Elementary l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 High School 9 10 ll 12 College I 2 3 4 Degree? Graduate School 5 6 7 8+ Degree? 6O AGAIN, A QUESTION ABOUT YOUR NATURAL (BIOLOGICAL) FATHER 7f. How would you describe his primary cultural/ethnic heritage? CIRCLE ONE (a) White (d) Native American (American Indian) (b) Black (e) Asian/Asian American (c) Hispanic/Hispanic-American (f) Other (describe) AQOUT YOUR NATURAL (BIOLOGICAL) HOTHER 8a. Where was she born? STATE COUNTRY--IF NOT U.S. ABOUT THE ADULT FEEALE WHO LIVED WITH YOU HOST OF THE TIHE UNTIL YOU WERE ID. (This could be your natural mother. or stepmother. or someone else). so. What kind of work did this adult female do (the adult female who lived with you most of the time until you were ID?) That is. what was her occupation? (For example: electrical engineer. file clerk. assembly line worker bookkeeper. sales clerk) Be. What were her most important activities or duties? (For example: keep account books. filing. selling clothing. teach fifth graders) 8d. What kind of business or industry was this? (For example: TV and radio mfg.. Retail shoe store. Automobile manufacturing [Oldsmobile]. State Labor Dept.) Be. What was the highest grade of school she completed? CIRCLE THE HIGHEST GRADE COMPLETED. None 0 Elementary l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 High School 9 10 ll 12 College l 2 3 4 Degree? Graduate School 5 6 7 8+ Degree? AQAIN. A QUESTION ABOUT YOUR NATURAL (BIOLOGICAL) HOTHE : Of. How would you describe her primary cultural/ethnic heritage? ClRCLE ONE (a) White (d) Native American (American indian) (b) Black (e) Asian/Asian American (c) Hispanic/Hispanic-American (f) Other (describe) 3 of 6 61 9a. Until you were 18. what religion was practiced in your home most of the time? CIRCLE ONE l. Protestant 2. Roman Catholic 3. Jewish 4. None. no religion 5. Other religion (please explain) 9b. What denomination? (please try to specify fully) 9c. Until you were 18. how often did you attend religious services? ClRCLE ONE 1. several times a week 2. about once a week 3. 2-3 times a month 4. once a month or less 5. never lOa. What is your religious preference 99!? CIRCLE ONE I. Protestant 2. Roman Catholic 3. Jewish 4. None. no religion 5. Other religion (please explain 10b. What denomination? (please try to specify fully) lOc. About how often did you attend religious services in the lastgyear? CIRCLE ONE l. several times a week 2. about once a week 3. 2-3 times a month 4. once a month or less 5. never 10d. Regardless of your attendance at religious services. how religious do you consider yourself to be? I. 2. 3. 4. not religious at all not very religious fairly religious very religious 4 of 6 62 ll. What was the highest grade you completed? CIRCLE THE HIGHEST GRADE COMPLETED. None 0 Elementary l 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 High School 9 ID II 12 Post High School Voc-Tech School I 2 3 College I 2 3 4 Degree? Graduate School 5 6 7 8+ Degree? I2a. What kind of work are you doing (what is your occupation)? (For example: electrical engineer. machinist. stock clerk. assembly line worker. teacher. farmer) 12b. What are your most important activities or duties? (For example: keep account books. filing. selling cars. operate printing press. finish concrete. teach fifth graders. answer phone). I2c. What kind of business or industry is this? (For example: TV and radio mfg.. Retail shoe store. Automobile manufacturing [Oldsmobile]. State Labor Dept.. Farm work) IZd. Are you: CHECK ONE an employee of a PRIVATE company. business or individual for wages. salary. or commission? a GOVERNMENT employee (federal. state. county. or local government? self-employed in OWN business. professional practice. or farm? own business not incorporated own business incorporated working WITHOUT pay in a family business or farm IZe. Approximately what is your present annual family income? CIRCLE ONE I. under 54.000 6. $16.00I--$20.000 2. S 4.001-$ 7.000 7. $20.001--S30.000 3. 8 7.001--310.000 8. 830.00I-$S0.000 4. $i0.001-813.000 9. $50.00l--875.000 5. $13.001--316.000 IO. over 375.000 5 of 6 I3. l4a. 14b. lSa. 15b. 63 How many times have you been married? CIRCLE ONE 0 l 2 3 4+ What was the date of your marriage to your (present) spouse? If married more than once. what was the date of your first marriage? List the children you have had from your present marriage or any previous marriages. Please list all children. starting with the oldest. and include birthdate. sex. and check ( '0 if the child lives with you now. FIRST NAHE ONLY BIRTHDATE SEX LIVING N91 (mo/day/Year) WITH YOU LIVING WITH NOW YOU NOW Now please circle the names of the children you listed in Question ISa above who are from your present marriage. If all are from your present marriage Just check a mark here . THANK YOU FOR FILLING OUT THIS QUESTIONNAIRE 6 of 6 Appendix C Diagnosdc Interview Schedule 64 DIAGNOSTIC INTERVIEW SCHEDULE DECK 01 Version III (5/81) Emily Nuuber: '0 CODE . . oooooooooo 3”” cm oooooooooo mu H” “mm oooooooooo mt: pasmmmpos: 3:3?“ i'v- 8888888888 .m: oooooooooo “ 00 ooooeooooo 888888M°° AM W ””“m"“' m ooooeooooo , OM ON 1. RECORD SEX A3 OBSERVED. 0 MALE ..................................... O FEMALE ................................... Q 131 2. Howoldwereyouonyourlasibinhdey? o . o oooooooo W “mma ooooeooooo a What is your oil-thesis? 06 ”WWE:] ggggoooooo 16/ “'[13 oooooooooo ma ooooeooooo [I] oooooooooo a Areyoupmentlymernedorareyouwidowed.seperammivoroedmrheveyouneverbeenmarned? Married .................................... <0 22/ Widowed ........ (sumi To a. s) .......... Q sow-Ice ....... (SKIP TO 0. s) .......... O Divan-o ........ (skip To 0. e) .......... G mm....IskIPTo 0.5) .......... O a Are you currently living with your (husband/wile)? No .............. (SKIP To 0. '7) .......... 0 2:1 m ............. (SKIP To 0. 7) .......... 0 a Anyoueumnllyuvingwiihsomeoneuthoughyouweremarried? No ........................................ 0 24/ IIF NEVER MARRIED. GO TO 0. 11 I 1 65 DECK 09 1 . Now i'd like to ask about your lite as a child. Let's begin with some questions about school. Did you ever repeat a grade? No .................. (SKIP To 0. 197)....0 in Yes ..................... (ASK A) ........ 0 A. Did you get held back more than once? No. only once ............................. 0 w Yes. more than once ....................... 3 16?. How were your grades in school-better than average. average. or not so good? Betterthaneverage ..(8KIP TO 0. 188).... 9 :s/ Average ............. (SKIP TO 0. 188).... 0 Notsogood ............. (ASK A) ........ 0 A. Did your teachers think youdidsboutsswell as youcouldordldtheythinkyouhadmeacilityto do much better? Didasweilascouid ..... (SKIP To a. its) 0 .0, Could have done much better .(ASK a) ..... 0 B. How old were you when your teachers lirst felt that way? an: *——‘ooooooooco 5333.33 .._._loooooooooo Lmnwewsn: if R SAYS ‘DK": ASK c] C. Do you think it was before you were 15 or later than that? Drums ...... (RECORD 01 ABOVE) 23/ 15 O! more ..... (RECORD es ABOVE) Still DK ...... (RECORD 88 ABOVE) 160 Did you frequently get into trouble with the teacher or principal tor misbehaving in school? (ELEMENTARY. JUNIOR HIGH. 08 HIGH SCHOOL) No ................. (SKIP To a. tee)....0 24/ Yes ..................... (ASK A) ......... 0 A. How old were you when you first got into trouble tor misbehaving in school? ENTEhAaes '— “0000000000 donates _JEoocooooooo liNTERVlEWER: lF R SAYS “DK': ASK B I I. Do you think it was beiore you were is or later than that? Under 15 ...... (RECORD 01 ABOVE) 27/ iSormore ..... (RECORD es ABOVE) Still DK ...... (RECORD es ABOVE) $1 66 188. Were you ever expelled or suspended lrorn school? (ELEMENTARY. JUNIOR HIGH OR HIGH _0 scuoou No ........ (SKIP TO O. 200) ............ G Yes ............ (ASK A) ................. 0 A. Howoid'wareyouwhenyouwereiirstexpelledorsuspended? ENTERAGEl‘——‘ 0000000000 oomozoo (0000000000 Wis.” B. DoyouthintltwasbeioreyouwereiSoriaterthenthat? Underts ..... (RECORD 01 ABOVE) iSormore ....(RECORD 6 ABOVE) Still DK ..... (escono as AaoVE) 280. Didyoueverolayhookyiromschooletieesttwicelnoneyear? DECK 09 31/ all No ........ (SKIP To 0. 201) ............. 0 Yes ........... (ASK A) .................. 0 A. Was that only in your last year in school or before that? Lastyearonly....(SKlP To 6. 201) ....... 0 Beiorelastyear ...(ASKaANo o) ........ 0 8. Did you play hookyas much as Sdayss year in at leasttwoachool years. not counting your last year in school? No ....................................... 0 Yes ................ . ..................... 0 C. Howoldwereyouwhenyoutlrstplayedhooky? ENTEhAaes' *000000000 complain [1.1 0 90 00000000 D. Doyouthlnkltwasbeioreyouwereiiorlaterthanthat? Under 15 ..... (RECORD 01 ABOVE) 15 ormore ....(RECORD 85 ABOVE) Still Dix ..... (RECORD fl ABOVE) 2°01. Dldyouevergetlntotroubleatschooitorilghtlng? No ............ (SKIP To a. sac) ......... 0 Yes ................ (ASK A) ............. 0 A. Did that happen more than once? No ............ (SKIP To 0. 202) ......... 0 Yea ............ (ASK E AND c.) .......... 0 as! 67 DECK 09 S. Were you sometimes the one who started the light? No ...................................... 0 so Yes ..................................... 0 C. How old were you when you first gOI into trouble for fighting at school? ENTERAGEII 0000000000 GOTOO.202 30000000000 “’ D. Do you think it was before you were 15 or later than that? Under 15 (RECORD 01 ABOVE) 43/ 15 or more (RECORD 85 ABOVE) Still DK (RECORD 85 ABOVE) 81 Before age 15. did you ever get into trouble with the police. your parents or neighbors because of fighting (other than for fighting at school)? No...(SKIP TO INSTRUCTIONS BEFORE O. 202E) 0 «I Yes ................... (ASK A) .................. 0 A Did that happen more than once? No ............. .(SKIP To c) ............ 0 45/ Yes ............... (ASK B) .............. 0 8. Were you sometimes the one who started the fight? N ...................................... 0 v; ..................................... 0 “I C. At what age did you first get into trouble because of fighting (away from school)? wenmemo—‘oocooooooo .1, SKIP TO O. 203: _J ,0000000000 D. Doyouthlnkitwasbeforey‘ouwere15orlatarthanthst? Under 15 ...(RECORD 01 ABOVE I SKlP TO 0. 203) eel 15 ormore .. (RECORD ’5 ABOVE l SKIP TO 0. 203) SUII DK ..(RECORD 88 ABOVE A SKIP TO 0. 203) INTERVIEW”: ARE BOTH O. 201 AND 202 CODED 1? E No ............ (SKIP TO 0. 203) ........ 0 .0) YES ................ (ASK E) ............. 0 E. Even though you didn't get into trouble for fighting. did you start fights more than once before you were 15? No ...................................... 0 st . Yes ..................................... 0 53 °§ 0.? 03 68 DECK oil When you were a kid. did you ever run away from home overnight? No ......... (SKIP To 0. 20s) ........... 0 am Yes ............. (ASK A) ................ 0 - A Did you run away more than once? No. just once ............................. 0 ss/ Yea.morethenonce ....................... 0 B. How old were ya when you first ran away from home overnight? ENTEAADEA‘__‘0000000000 ooroo.zoi,_) “’ _JOOOOOOOOOO C. Doyouthlnkltwesbeforeyouwereisorlaterthsnthat? Under 15 ....(RECORD 01 ABOVE) ss/ 15 ormore ...(RECORD BS ABOVE) Still DK .(RECORD BB ABOVE) Ofcourse.nooneteilsthetruthalthetime.butdidyoutellslotoflieswhenyouwereschildor teenager? No .......... (SKIP To 0. 205) .......... 0 s71 Yes .............. (ASK A) .. . .~ ........... 0 A How old were you when you first told a lot of lies? ENTERAGEA F— 0000000000 OOTOO.2osl_E|0000000000- 5" B. DoyouthinltitwasbaforeyouwerelSorlaterthanB'Iat? Under 15 ....(RECORD 01 ABOVE) as/ 15 ormore ...(RECORD 85 ABOVE) Still DK ....(RECORD SB ABOVE) Whenyouweraschlld.didyoumorethan onceswipe thingsfromstoresortromotherchlldrenorstssl from your parents or from anyone else? No .......... (SKIP To a. zoo) ......... ..0 e11 Yes .............. (ASK A).......- ......... 0 A. Howoldwereyouwhanyoufirststolethlngs? ENTERAdEa‘ _'0000000000 GOTOO.205E_0000000000 W liNTERVIEWER: iF R SAYS "DK": A§K El 69 8 Do you think II was before you were 15 or later than that? Under 15 ..... (RECORD 01 ABOVE) 15 or more . . . . (RECORD 95 ABOVE) StIll DK ..... (RECORD DB ABOVE) DECK 08 BEGIN DECK 10 206. When you were a kid. did you ever intentionally damage someone's car or do anything else to destroy or 200. O severely damage someone else's property? No ........... (SKIP To 0. 207) .......... 0 Yes ............... (ASK A) ............... 0 A How old were you when you first did that? ENTERAoeal_—'0000000000 cOToo.207_.J_.0000000000 limggyggggn; IF R §AY§ 2K": A§K E I B. Doyouminkltwasbsioreyouwere15orlaterthantriet? Under 15 ..... (RECORD O1 ABOVE) 15 or more ....(RECORD BS ABOVE) Still DK ..... (RECORD BB ABOVE) . Were you ever arrested as a juvenile or sent to Invenile court? No ........... (SKIP TO 0. 208) ........ ..0 Yes ............... (ASK A) .............. .9 A. HOW Old ”'0 you the first (IMO? ENTEAAse:‘——‘oocooooooo cDTOO.200,._J__0000000000 (INTERVIEWER: iF R SAYS "OK": ASK BI B. Do you think it was before you were 15 or later than that? Under 15 ..... (RECORD 01 ABOVE) 15 ormore ....(RECORD 85 ABOVE) Still DK .....(RECORD fl ABOVE) Have you ever been arrested since 15 for anything other than traffic violations? No ........... (SKIP TO O. 209) ..... ....0 Yes ...........(ASKAAND a) ------- ---0 A Have you been arrested more than once? NOJUBIOM eases eeeeeeee eeeeeeeeaeeeeeeeQ . Yes.morethsnonce fl: 0 B. Have you ever been convicted of a felon” No ................................... ....0 Yes 3 Q. "I 12! 131 15/ 15! 17/ 1V 7O coo: DECK 10 1 e 09 M ”D 2 - asto- cut 5 0 yes a - enags or are 208. Have you had at least four traffic BcKets in your life for speeding or running a light or causing an O accident? No ...................................... 0 18' VB. :0. 210. Now l'm going to ask you about your sexual experience. in general. has your sex life been important to O you.orcouidyouhavegottenslongsswellwithoutlt? Somewhat important ..................... 0 av Gotten along as well without it ............ 0 No sexual experience ................... . . 0 SWIM USD: 11' NO SE! EDERIDICE OI REFUSES an QUESTIONS. am 1‘0 Q. 523. ID SUPPLDENT: U IO SE! mmDICE, “I? TO Q. 215. If REFUSES 5E1 DESTIGS SE1? TO CTIMS ABOVE . 216. 61:. Has having sexual relations ever been physically painful for you? MD: - SELF: 9 Q 9 Q 21/ 212. Has there been a period of several months in your life when having sex was not pleasurable for you (even when it wasn't painful)? MD: SELF: 0 Q 0 Q Q a! 213. Have you had any (other) kind of sexual difficulties (FOR MEN. such as a period 0 of two months or more when you had trouble having an erection)? up: SELF: 00000 as: INSERT SUPPLEMENT HERE 217. Since age 15. have you been in more than one fightthat came to swapping blows (other than fights with 0 your [husband/wifelpsrtnerl)? [INTEItvIswuc IF a VOLUNTEERS -ONLY AS REQUIRED sY JOB." coo: H No .......................... 0 Yes A 0' w V 85. HaveyouevermamlikesstictJtnifmorgun.inafightsincsyouwers15? - INTERVIEWER: IF R SAYS ‘ONLY AS REQUIRED BY OCCUPATION.” CODE 1 IF R SAYS 'WIELDED BUT MISSED." CODE 5 NO eaeeeeaeeeee eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee G ‘B/ V. :w. 618. °§ °S 05! 71 ceDt DECK 11 l o no - d - med see 1 0 aero- enl 5 . yes 5 0 drugs or sit Since you were 15. did you ever hold (three or more different lobawithln a five-year period? INTERVIEWER: OMIT CHANGES VOLUNTEERED AS DUE TO JOB ENDING. RETURN TO SCHOOL. iLLNESS OR MATERNITY. TRANSFER OF SPOUSE. BECOMING HOUSEWIFE. COUNT CHANGES IN MAIN JOB ONLY. NO................................; ..... 0 . 1" Yes +0 v- Have you beenfimsfrom more than one job? No ................................. .....0 . 17/ YO. 4:0 . Since you were 15. have you an. bursa times or more before you already had another he lined up? INTERVIEWBR: OMIT OUITTING VOLUNTEERED AS DUE TO RETURN TO SCHOOL. 1 ILLNESS OR MATERNITY. TRANSFER OF SPOUSE. BECOMING HOUSEWIFE. No ....................................... Q . w Y” :0 On any job you have had since you were 15. were you late or absehtan average of 5 days a month or more? umaviewss: IF it SAYS "NO SET SCHEDULE”: CODE fl RECORD ALC/MED: 0 Q . 15/ #0 How many months out of die last.fiva years have you been without a job? ENTERIMONTHS:F"_'0000000000 10' .L._u.0000000000 liNTERVIEVER: IF LESS THAN 0 MOS. SKIP TO 0. mj IF 5 MOS. OR MORE. ASK B IF R SAYS ”OK.” ASK A. A Doyouthinltltwssisssthathonthsormorethanthat? Less than 5 mos. ..... (RECORD OI ABOVE S SKIP TO O. 224) w 5 mos. or more ...... (RECORD 85 ABOVE l ASK B) Still OK ............. (RECORD DB ABOVE A ASK B) B. For how much of that time did you want to wont but were not able to find a job? ENTER'MONTHS:l—T_' 0000000000 3’ _L_ 0000000000 LNTERVIEWER: IF s - s MOS. on MORE. SKIP TO F] a: 223. OE 05! 72 DECK ii (Continued) C. for how much of that time were you not looking for work because of emotional or mental problems or because of problems with drugs or alcohol? ENTEROMONTHS:'——@®@Q®@@@@@ ss/ [_JJcooooooooo [INTERVIEWERz IF 5 . c - s MOS. OR MORE. SKIP To EI D. How much time (besides that) were you Just not interested in working but not in school. or physically ill (or retired or a housewife)? ENTER MONTHS;‘_'—‘0000000000 , ' ._O_Ji0000000000 ” E. HTERVIE‘WER: DO B s C t D O B MONTHSOR MOREleIHOUT ,WORK? NO ...................................... O 31 YES 4, 0" Haveyoueverusadanaliasoranassumedname? WmVIEY/ER: IF R VOLUNTEERS ONLY PEN on STAGE NAME- cooE 1.| No ...................................... 0 . 30/ YO! - t 0 Have you thought that you lied, pretty often since you have been an adult? NO ..................................... . G 31/ YBB ..................................... Have you ever traveled around for a month or more without having any arrangements ahead of time and net knowing how long you were going to stay or where you were going to work? [ lNTERVleER: IF R VOLUNTEERS "ONLY ON VACATION FROM JOB”: cooaj No ...................................... 0 w YRS f G. Hasthereeverbean s periodwhanyouhadnoregulsrpiacetotive. for at Ieastamonthorso? N ...... ............................ 0 Y; :0‘ w 73 DECK 11 INTERVIEWER: DOES R HAVE CHILD (O. 14 I 01 OR MORE) OR E HAS A AcTED As PARENT (D. 14A - 5)? ..(SKIP TO INSTRUCTIONS BEFORE O. 202) .............. 0 YES ....... (AS): 0. 220) ................ o a.) . Have you sometimes left young diiidreIRunder 5 years did home alone while you were shOpping or out doing anyming else? * lNTERVlswslt: IF R VOLUNTEERS -ONLY IN ENEROENOY: "FOR LESS THAN so MINUTES.' OR -CI-llLD COULD sE HEARD OR COULD cows THERE-z cops 1. No . . . ....... . ........ . .................. 0 . a. Yes :0 m "'00 VIE! CHIC ............... (SKIP TO INSTRUCTIONS BEFORE O. 232) ....... 0 Have there been times when a neighbufedaail'd'mfyours/youwere caring for) because you didnt ' get around to snapping for food or cocking. or keptyour child overnight because no one was taking care of him at home? FINTERVIEwER: IF R VOLUNTEERS “ONLY IN EMERGENCY”: cooefi No ...................................... 0 ss/ Yes ; Q. . Has a nurse. or social worker or tssctlsr ever said that any child (of yourslyou were caring for) wasn't being givanenouul beater wasn’t being kept clam encugh or wasnt getting medial carewhen it was needed? No ... ........... . ....................... 0 37/ money-onyourselforongoingout? 74 INTERVIEWER; I-low MANY ‘5"s" HAVE BEEN CODED IN Os. 200.231 (BEGINNING ON PAGE 55)? LL NONE. ONE OR Two ..(SKIP TO O. 235) 0 TNREE OR MORE ...... (ASK O. 232) ..... 0 ... 232. Did you ever talk to a doctor about any of these things you did like (SPECIFY '5"s" WHICH HAVE 8:. Didyoudosny'ofth'ssedlingsbstwesntheageaofuanda? 0.? BEEN CODED. BEGINNING WITH 0. 200. p. 55) NO ...................................... 0 a Yes ................................. 0 — INTERVIEWER: IS R OLDER OR YOUNGER THAN 25 YEARS OLD? E ZBOROLDER ........ (ASK 0. sea) ...... 0 YOUNGER THAN25 .. (SKIP TO o. 204) . 0 41/ No .......... (ASK A) ............... - ..... 0 42/ Yes ....(SKIP TO 0. 204) ............... 0 A Was there some reason yOU couldn‘t have done these things between 15 and 25. for instance. because yOu were ill in bed dist whole time (or in jail/not married/had no children)? No ....(I-IAO OPPORTUNITY) ............ 0 43/ Yes ...(NO OPPORTUNITY) ............. 0 When is the last time you did any one of these things like (MENTION CODED '5"s" BEGINNING WITH 0. 205)? _ MthinlsstZweeks ......... '. ............ .8 “I CODE “'05" WWW”‘Ii'IIIIIIIIIIIIIII'o b RECENT "”5 Withinisstysar ....... ..0 POSSIBLE a Withlnlastayesrs ......... ...............0 Hommnayesrsago ....... .(ASK A)....0 I MORE THAN 2 YEARS ADD: A Howpldwsreyouthelasttlmsyoudidanyofthossthings? ENTERAGE'DJO000000000 45/ 0000000000 W5... . Appendix D Drinking and Drug History Form 75 (nfg:mgtiog_gg_ngigkigg gng Other Drug Us; R Number: (6/I/B9) (I3 pages) Given By: Date: Ans. Chk: This questionnaire takes about IS minutes to complete. All information will be used for research only and will be kept strictly confidential. If you are not sure Of the answer to a question please answer the best you can. Please try to answer each item. A. THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS ARE ABOUT YOUR DRINKING OF ALCOHOLIC BEVERAGES: I. HOW OLD WERE YOU THE FIRST TIHE YOU EVER TOOK A DRINK? DO NOT COUNT THE TIMES WHEN YOU WERE GIVEN A "SIP” BY AN ADULT. years Old. 2. OVER THE LAST 6 QQNTHS. ON THE AVERAGE. HOW HANY DAYS A MONTH HAVE YOU HAD A DRINK? days a month. 3. OVER THE LAST §_fiQflIfl§. ON A DAY WHEN YOU ARE DRINKING. HOW HANY DRINKS DO YOU USUALLY HAVE IN 24 HOURS? (A DRINK IS A I2 02. CAN. GLASS OR BOTTLE OF BEER: A 4 OZ. GLASS OF WINE: A SINGLE SHOT; OR A SINGLE 'HIXED DRINK.”) drinks per 24 hours. 4. OVER THE PAST 6 MONTHS. WHEN YOU GOT DRUNK. HOW BAD WAS YOUR HANGOVER? Never bad ______ Pretty Bad Not bad Terrible A little less than average Worst possible Average Never drank enough to get A little more than average hangover CONTINUE ON THE NEXT PAGE. Page l of l3 765 . THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS ARE ABOUT YOUR DRINKING PATTERNS. IN ANSWERING THE QUESTIONS. PLEASE TNIIK ABOUT WHAT YOU HAVE DONE o__N_ THE AVERAGE OVER THE i_._A§T Six NONTNS. WHEN ommw. a. liOii OFTEN DO YOU USUALLY HAVE WINE OR A PUNCH CONTAINING WINE? 3 or more times a day 2 or 3 times a month 2 times a day About once a month Once a day Less than once a month. Nearly every day but at least once a year 3 or 4 times a week Less than once a year once or twice a week NEVER [If checked. go to question 52a] b. THINK OF ALL THE TIRES YOU HAD WINE OR A PUNCH CONTAINING WINE RECENTLY. WHEN YOU DRINK WINE. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE ID OR MORE GLASSES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Nore than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION #2 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER c. WHEN YOU DRINK WINE. llOil OFTEN 00 You HAVE As NANY AS 7 TO 9 GLASSES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Nora than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER 6. WHEN YOU DRINK WINE. NOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE AS HANY AS 5 to 6 GLASSES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Nore than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER e. WHEN YOU DRINK WINE. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE AS HANY AS 3 to 4 GLASSES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Nore than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION 52 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER 2 of l3 '77 F. WHEN YOU DRINK WINE. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE I TO 2 GLASSES? Nearly every time Nora than half the time Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER WHEN DRINKING BEER 0. HOW OFTEN DO YOU USUALLY HAVE BEER? 3 or more times a day 2 or 3 times a month About once a month Less than once a month. but at least once a year Less than once a year NEVER [If checked. go to question 53a] 2 times a day Once a day Nearly every day 3 or 4 times a week Once or twice a week b. THINK OF ALL THE TIRES YOU HAD BEER RECENTLY. WHEN YOU DRINK BEER. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE ID OR KORE CANS. GLASSES OR BOTTLES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 53 BELOW Hore than half the time: SKIP T0 QUESTION 53 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER ' c. WHEN YOU DRINK BEER. NOll OFTEN DO YOU HAVE AS NANY As 7 To 9 CANS. GLASSES OR BOTTLES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 53 BELOW hore than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION #3 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER d. WHEN YOU DRINK BEER. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE AS NANY AS 5 T0 6 CANS. GLASSES OR BOTTLES? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION 53 BELOW Nore than half the time: SKIP TD QUESTION 53 BELOW Less than half the time ' Once in a while NEVER 3 of l3 78 e. WHEN YOU DRINK BEER. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE AS HANY AS 3 to 4 CANS. GLASSES OR BOTTLES? Nearly every time: SKIP T0 QUESTION Q3 BELOW Kore than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION #3 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while AEVER f. WHEN YOU DRINK BEER. HOW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE I TO 2 CANS. GLASSES OR BOTTLES? Nearly every time Nore than half the time Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER WHEN ORINKIQQ WHISKEY OR LIQUOR a. HOW OFTEN DO YOU USUALLY RAVE WHISKEY OR LIQUOR (SUCH AS HARTINIS. HANHATTANS. HIGHBALLS. OR STRAIGHT DRUMS iNCLUDlNG SCOTCH. BOURBON. GIN. VODKA. RUN. ETC.)2 3 or more times a day 2 or 3 times a month About once a month Less than once a month. Nearly every day but at least once a year 3 or 4 times a week Less than once a year Once or twice a week EVER [If checked. no to question “I 2 times a day Once a day b. mm: or ALL THE mes YOU mo DRIiKS COITAININO :04:st: OR OTHER LIQUOR neccmu. men YOU HAVE HAD THEN. How OFTEN 00 YOU HAVE no on now: claims: Nearly every time: SKIP TO OLESTION '4 BELOW Kore than half the time: SKIP TO QlESTION #4 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while iEVER c. WHEN YOU HAVE HAD WIMS CWTAINING WHISKEY G? OTl'ER LIQDR. W “TEN DO YOU HAVE AS HANY AS 7 TO 9 DRUMS? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QIESTION .4 BELOW Bore than half the time: SKIP TO OLESTION #4 KLOW Less than half the time Once in a while EVER AUIS U 79 d. WHEN YOU HAVE HAD DRIMS CONTAINING WHISKEY a? OTHER LIQLDR. MW OFTEN DO YOU HAVE AS HANY AS 5 TO 6 DRIMS? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTION .4 BELOW Kore than half the time: SKIP TO QUESTION (4 BELOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER e. WHEN YOU HAVE HAD DRIMS WAININ WISKEY m LIQLm. l'DW NTEN no you HAVE 3 TO A DRIVKS? Nearly every time: SKIP TO QUESTICRI t4 BELOW Here than half the time: SKIP TO QLESTION #4 KLOW Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER f. WHEN YOU HAVE HAD DRI'KS CONTAINING WHISKEY (I? LIQUOR. i'DW NTEN DO YOU HAVE I TO 2 DRIMS? Nearly every time More than half the time Less than half the time Once in a while NEVER I I .CI‘ECKl'OdOFTENYOU HAVE ANYDRII’K CONTAINING ALm. WETHER IT IS WINE. BEER. WISKEY OR ANY OTI'ER DRIM. HAKE SURE THAT YOUR ANSWER IS MT LESS FREMNT THAN THE FREQIENCY REPORTED CW ANY OF THE PRECEDING QJESTIUIS. Once or twice a week 2 or 3 times a month About once a month Less than once a month. but at least once a year Less than once a year 3 or more times a day 2 times a day Once a day Nearly every day 3 or 4 times a week Now a Question about earlier in your lifg: l‘DW OLD WERE YOU THE FIRST TINE YOU EVER DRAM EIDUGH TO GET DRUM? years old. Sofl3 80 6a. WE ARE ALSO INTERESTED IN THE OCCASIONS THAT HAY BE RARE (OR NOT). WHEN PEOPLE DRINK A LOT HORE THAN THEY USUALLY DO. I! THE LAST SIX HONTHS. THINK OF THE 24 HOUR PERIOD WHEN YQQ DID Iflfl HOST DRINKINQ: THIS WOULD BE A DAY SOHEWHERE iN THE PERIOD BETWEEN . AND NOW. (month) (year) On that day. how many drinks did you have? (A drink is a (2 oz. can. bottle. or glass of beer. a 4 oz. glass of wine. a single shot. or a single mixed drink). 30 or more drinks 25 - 29 drinks 20 - 24 drinks IS - l9 drinks l0 - I4 drinks 7 — 9 drinks 5 - 6 drinks 3 - 4 drinks I — 2 drinks none 6b. APPROXIHATELY WHEN DID THIS HAPPEN? . . (month) (year) .6c. Now ANSWER THIS QUESTION FOR W W- W “*1 24 m PERIOD W DRINKING. HOW HANY DRINKS DID YOU HAVE? 3D or more drinks 25 - 29 drinks 20 - 24 drinks I5 - l9 drinks in - I4 drinks 7 - 9 drinks 5 - 6 drinks 3 - 4 drinks I - 2 drinks none 6d. APPROXIHATELY WHEN DID THIS HAPPEN? 9 (month) (Vlmr) 6 of I3 )0. I). )2. III ANSWER KEY FOR QUESTIONS BELOW: I 2 3-5 6-l0 11-20 21-50 Sl-IOO 301-250 251-500 SDI-I000 I000+ (more than 1000) . C. NOW SOHE QUESTIONS ABOUT OUTCOHES PEOPLE SOHETIHES HAVE BECAUSE OF DRINKING. HAVE YOU EVER HAD ANY OF THE FOLLOWING HAPPEN BECAUSE OF YOUR DRINKING? 1§§ 59 HOW ANY AGE AGE (check one) TIHES first most (approx.- time recent see key)“ time Hissed school or time on Job Thought I was drinking too much Gone on a binge of constant drinking for 2 or more days Lost friends My spouse or others in my Family (my parents or children) objected to my drinking Felt guilty about my drinking Divorce or separation ToOk a drink or two first thing in morning Restricted my drinking to certain times of day or week in order to control it or cut down. (like after SPH. or only on weekends. or only with other people) Been fired or laid off Once started drinking, kept on going till completely intoxicated Had a car accident when l was driving _—:i-i;: ' SELECT YOUR ANSWER FRO" KEY AT THE TOP OF THE PAGE Questions continue on the next Page. 7 of i3 82 I3. 14. I7. I9. 20. 2‘. 22. ANSER KEY FQ ggg‘rig gig: i 2 3-5 6-10 ll-20 Zl-SO 5i-I00 l0I-250 251-500 50i-l000 I000+ (more than I000) HOW NANY ADE AGE TINES first most 1§§ N9 approx- time recent (check one) see key)' time Kept on drinking after i promised myself not to Had to go to a hospital (other than accidents) Had to stay in a hospital overnight Had the shakes “the morning after” Heard or saw or felt things that weren't there. hallucinations) several days after stopping drinking Had blackouts (couldn't rail-ember later what you'd done while drinking) Been given a ticket fer drunk driving (DWI or DUIL) Had a Jerking or fits (convulsions) several days after stopping drinking Been given a ticket for public intoxication. drunk and disorderly. or other nondrlving alcohol arrest Had the D.T.'s (delirium tremens. shakes. sweating. rapid heart. etc.) within 2 - 3 days after stopping drinking ‘ SELECT ANSWERS FROH THE KEY AT THE TOP OF THE PAGE B OF 13 83 D. THE LAST SECTIONS OF THIS QUESTIONNAIRE DEAL WITH VARIOUS DRUGS OTHER THAN ALCOHOL. THERE IS STILL A LOT OF TALK THESE DAYS ABOUT THIS SUBJECT. BUT VERY LITTLE ACCURATE INFORHATION. PARTICULARLY ABOUT PATTERNS OF USE OF THESE SUBSTANCES IN ADULTHOOD. THEREFORE. WE STILL HAVE A LOT TO LEARN ABOUT THE ACTUAL EXPERIENCES OF PEOPLE YOUR AGE. WE HOPE THAT YOU CAN ANSWER ALL QUESTIONS: BUT IF YOU FIND ONE WHICH YOU FEEL YOU CANNOT ANSWER HONESTLY. WE WOULD PREFER THAT YOU LEAVE IT BLANK. REHEHBER THAT YOUR ANSWERS WILL BE KEPT STRICTLY CONFIDENTIAL AND THEY ARE NEVER CONNECTED WITH YOUR NAME. THAT IS WHY THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS IDENTIFIED ONLY WITH A CODE NUMBER. THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS ARE ABOUT CIGARETTES (CHECK THE BEST ANSWER): la. HAVE YOU EVER SHOKED CIGARETTES? Never (60 TO QUESTION 3) Once or twice Occasionally but not regularly Regularly in the past Regularly now lb. HAVE YOU SMOKED CIGARETTES DURING THE PAST 12 MONTHS? Never (60 TO QUESTION 3) Once or twice Occasionally but not regularly Regularly for a while during this year. but not now Regularly now 2. HOW FREQUENTLY HAVE YOU SMOKED CIGARETTES DURING THE PAST 30 DAYS? Not at all Less than one cigarette per day One to five cigarettes per day About one-half pack per day About one pack per day About one and one-half packs per day Two packs or more per day Will E. THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS ARE ALL ABOUT NON-PRESCRIPTION USE OF DRUGS. E THER FOR RECREATION OR FOR SELF-MEDICATION. é (HARK ONE SPACE FOR EACH LINE). 3. ON HOW HANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU USED HARIJUANA (GRASS. POT) 0R HASHISH (HASH. HASH OIL) More than 1000 I g g 10~19 Occasions 0 Occasions 1-2 Occasions In your lifetime? ( During the last l2 months? During the last 30 days? ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( wv AA wv AA vv AA v” AA VV AA «v AA vv A» vv AA haw \J (HARK ONE SPACE FOR EACH LINE). 4. ON HOW NANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU USED LSD (ACID) In your lifetime? During the last i2 months? During the last 30 days? 5. ON HOW HANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU USED PSYCHEDELICS OTHER THAN LSD (LIKE HESCALINE. PEYOTE. PSILOCYBIN. PCP) In your lifetime? During the last I2 months? During the last 30 days? 6. ON HOW HANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU USED COCAINE (COKE OR CRACK) In your lifetime? During the past 12 months? During the last 30 days? 7. AHPHETAHINES ARE SOHETIHES PRESCRIBED BY DOCTORS TO HELP PEOPLE LOSE WEIGHT OR TO GIVE PEOPLE HORE ENERGY. THEY ARE SONETIHES CALLED UPPERS. UPS. SPEED. CRYSTAL. CRANK. BENNIES. DEXIES. PEP PILLS. AND DIET PILLS. ON HOW HANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU TAKEN AHPHETAHINES ON YOUR OWN--THAT IS. WITHOUT A DOCTOR TELLING YOU TO TAKE THEN In your lifetime? During the last I2 months? During the last 30 days? Occasions 0 l-ZlOccasions 1-2 Occasions 3-5 Occasions 3-5 Occasions '10 of 13 6-9 Occasions 6-9 Occasions 10-19 Occasions 10-19 Occasions 10-19 Occasions 10-19 Occasions 20—39 Occasions 20-39 Occasions 40-99 Occasions 40—99 Occasions 100.1000 Occasion: 100-1000 Occasions More than 1000 More than 1000 More than 1000 More than 1000 (HARK ONE SPACE FOR EACH LINE). 8. ON How HANY OCCASIONS (If ANY) HAVE YOU USED QUAALUDES g (QUADS. SOAPERS. HETHAQUALONE) ... ON YOUR OWN-- THAT IS. WITHOUT A DOCTOR TELLING YOU TO TAKE THEN o In your lifetime? ( ) During the last ( l 12 months? During the last 30 days? ( ) 9. BARBITURATES ARE SOHETIHES PRE- SCRIBED BY DOCTORS TO HELP PEOPLE RELAX OR GET TO SLEEP. THEY ARE SOMETIMES CALLED DOWNS. DOWNERS. GOOFBALLS. YELLOWS. REDS. BLUES. RAINBOWS. ON HOW HANY OCCASIONS g (IF ANY) HAVE YOU TAKEN «4 BARBITURATES ON YOUR OWN -- 3 THAT IS. WITHOUT A DOCTOR 8 TELLING YOU To TAHE THEN o In your lifetime? ( During the last I2 months? During the last 30 days? ( I0. TRANQUILIZEPS ARE SOHETIHES PRESCRIBED BY DOCTORS TO CALH PEOPLE DOWN. QUIET THEIR NERVES. OR RELAX THEIR HUSCLES. LIBRIUH VALIUH. AND NILTOWN ARE ALL TRANQUILIZERS. ON HOW HANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) 3 HAVE YOU TAKEN TRANQUILIZERS 3 ON YOUR OWN -- THAT Is. WITHOUT A DOCTOR TELLING YOU 8 TO TAKE THEN o In your lifetime? ( During the last ( l2 months? During the last 30 days? ( 1—2 Occasions 85 1-2 Occasions 3~5 Occasions II Of 13 6—9 Occasions 10—19 Occasions A 10-19 Occasions 10-19 Occasions 20-39 Occasions 20—39 Occasions 40-99 Occasions " 40-99 Occasions 40-99 Occasions 100-1000 Occasions T‘T‘ 100-1000 Occasions A loo-1000 Occasions More than 1000 More than 1000 More than 1000 (BARK ONE SPACE FOR EACH LINE). II. ON HOW "ANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU USED HEROIN (SHACK. HORSE. SKAG) In your lifetime? ( During the last ( I2 months? During the last 30 days? ( I2. THERE ARE A NUHBER OF NARCOTICS OTHER THAN HEROIN. SUCH AS NETH- ADONE. OPIUH. HORPHINE. CODEINE. DENEROL. PAREGORIC. TALWIN. AND LAUDANUH. THESE ARE SOHETIHES PRESCRIBED BY DOCTORS. ON HOW MANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU TAKEN NARCOTICS OTHER THAN HEROIN ON YOUR OWN-- THAT IS. WITHOUT A DOCTOR TELLING YOU TO TAKE THEN In your lifetime? ( During the last ( 12 months? During the last 30 days? ( ‘3. ON HOW MANY OCCASIONS (IF ANY) HAVE YOU SNIFFED GLUE. OR BREATHED THE CONTENTS OF AEROSOL SPRAY CANS. OR INHALED ANY OTHER GASES OR SPRAYS IN 'ORDER TO GET HIGH 0 Occasions 0 Occasions In your lifetime? ( During the last ( I2 months? During the last 30 days? ( ’1-2 Occasions 86 Si .3 § 3 8 E 1?. l I I l I ) I I) I) .§ .5. U) I) g 8 8 E 3 3-5 Occasions 12 of 13 6-9 Occasions 6-9 Occasions 10-19 Occasions 10—19i0ccasions 10-19 Occasions 20—39 Occasions 20-39 Occasions 40-99 Occasions 40—99 Occasions 40-99 Occasions 100~1000 Occasb 100b1000 Occaéions . 100-1000 Occas. More than 1000 More than 1000 Here than 1000 87 F. NOW SOHE OTHER QUESTIONS ABOUT NONPRESCRIPTION USE OF DRUGS. HAVE YOU EVER HAD ANY OF THE FOLLOWING OUTCOHES BECAUSE OF YOUR USE OF THE NONPRESCRIPTION DRUGS ASKED ABOUT IN SECTION E (THE LAST SECTION)? ANSWER KEY FOR QUESTIONS BELOW: ' I 2 3-5 6-10 ”-20 21-50 51-100 101-250 251-500 500+ (more than 500) 1§§ N9 HOW HANY AGE AGE TIHES first most recent (approx) TIHE TIHE (see key)‘ I. Hissed school or time on Job Lost friends 3. Been divorced or separated 4. Been fired Or laid off 5. Had a car accident when you were driving 6. Had to go to a hospital (other than accidents) 7. Had to stay in hospital overnight 8. Had to see a doctor because of drug use (unintentional overdose) or had a doctor say drugs had harmed your health 9. Gone through physical with- drawal from drugs IO. Been arrested for possession or sale of drugs other than marijuana _—“--—--———————{T:TSELECT YOUR ANSWER FROH KEY AT THE TOP OF THE PAGET] Ila. Have you ever taken drugs intravenously (using a needle)? Don't count shots you were given by a doctor or nurse or shots you may have taken for treatment of diabetes. NO YES Ilb. IF YES. WHAT DRUGS HAVE YOU TAKEN INTRAVENOUSLY (IV)? IIC. AT WHAT AGE DID YOU FIRST TAKE AN IV DRUG? years old. lld. AT WHAT AGE WAS THE HOST RECENT TINE? years old. 13 0F 13 List of References 88 List of References Allen, D. L. Petersen, D. R., Wilson, J. R., McCleam, G. E. and Nishimoto, T. K. (1983). Selective breeding for a multivariate index of ethanol dependence in mice. results from the first five generations WW Researchl. 443- 337 American Psychiatric Association (1987). b 2.. '4; .. ' ' ' ° . a I :. WW Edition-Revised). Washington, D. C. A. P. A. Beck, AT and Beck, R. W. (1972) Screening depressed patients in family practice: A rapid technique WM? 81 ~85 Begleiter, H., Porjesz, J., Bihari, B. and Kissin, B. (1984). Event-related brain potentials in boys at risk for alcoholism. M 1493-1496. Bohman, M. (197 8). Some 6g9en2eti6c aspects of alcoholism and criminality. mm Wham. 2 - 7 - Bohman, M. Sigvardsson, S. and Cloninger, C. R. (1981). Maternal inheritance of alcohol abuse: Cross- -fostering analysis Of adopted women. W W 265 -269 Buchsbaum, M. S. Landau, S., Murphy, D. and Goodwin, F. (1973) Average evoked response in bipolar and unipolar affective disorders: Relationship to sex, age of onset and monoamine oxidase. WM, 199- 212. Cadoret, R. J. and Gath, A. ( 1978) Inheritance of alcoholism 1n adoptees. British WM 252- 258 Cadoret, R. J. O'Gorman, T. W., Troughton, E. and Haywood, E. (1985). Alcoholism and antisocial personality: Interrelationshipz, genetic and environmental factors. WM 161 1 7 Cadoret, R. J. Troughton, E. and O'Gorman, T. W. (1987). Genetic and environmental factors In alcohol abuse and antisocial personality. W AlthLAfil-8 89 Caetano, R. (1976). Two versions of dependence: DSM-III and the Alcohol Dependence Syndrome DmniAlCOmLDenendensoJi 81- 103 Cahalan, D., Cisin,l. and Crossley, H. (1969). Center of Alcohol Studies. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers Cahalan, D. and C1s1n, I (1976) Drinlo'ng behavior and drinking problems 1n the United States. In B. Kissin and H. Begleiter (Eds. ) GLOALSmjaLAstOmLAICDhOism. New York: Plenum Press Clark, W. B. and Cahalan, D. Changgs 1n problem drinking over a four-year span. Addimflehaximl 251 25 Cloninger, C. R. (1987). Neurogenic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. M, 410-416. Cloninger, C. R., Bohman, M. and Sigvardsson, S. (1981). Inheritance of alcohol abuse: Cress-fostc’fing analysis of adopted men MW 861- 868. Cloninger, C. R. Bohman, M, Sigvardsson, S. and VonKnorring, A- L. (1985). Psychopathology 1n adopted-out children of alcoholics: The Stockholm Adoption Study In M Galanter (Ed) W New York. Plenum Press. Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S. and Bohman, M. (1988). Childhood personality Esmrimtal. predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. W W 494-505 Cotton, N. S. (1979). 8The familial incidence of alcoholism: A review. mm mm. 9-116. Crabbe, J. C., Kosobud, A. and Young, E. R. (1983). Genetic selectiOn for ethanol gvuhdrawal severity: Differences 1n replicate mouse lines. W, 995- 62 Edwards, G. and Gross, M. M. (1976). W, 1058. Endicott, J. and Spitzer, R. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview. ALEIILELQLQQM Husband: 837- 844 90 Faraj,B., Lenton,J., Kutner,M., Camp, V., Stammers,T., Lee, S. R, Lolies,P. and Chandora, D. (1987). Prevalence of low monoamine oxidase function 1n alcoholism WWW. 464-468 Feighner, J., Robins, B., Guze, 8., Woodruff, R., Winokur, G. and Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. W W 57- 63 Fillmore. K M (1987) WOW W Unpublished manuscript. Gabrielli, W., Mednick, S., Volavka, J., Pollock, V., Schulsin er, F. and Itile, T. (1982). Electroencephalographs in children of alcoholics athers. W. 12, 404-407. Gabrielli, W. and Plomin, R. (1985). Drinking behavior 1n the Colorado Adoptee and Twin Sample loumalOSmdiesmflthAfi 24-31 Gallaher, E. J. and Gionet, S. E. (1988) Initial sensitivity and tolerance to ethanol 1n mice genetically selected fm dimmgensitivity. W W Goodwin, D. W. (1979). Alcoholism and Heredity. A review and a hypothesis. Archives WM 57-62 Goodwin, D. W., Schulsinger, F., Moller, N., Hermansen, L. ,Guze, S. and Winokur, G. (1973). Alcohol lems 1n adopwes raised apart from alcoholic biological parents. Archives cg 32:19:31 Esyghiamy. 28. 238- 243. Goodwin, D. W., Schulsinger, F. Moller, N. Hermansen, L. Winokur, G. and Guze, S.(1974). Drinking problems 1n adopét‘ed agd non-adopted sons of alcoholics. l -16 Helzer, J. Robins, L., McEvoy, L., Spitnagel, B., Stolzmann, R., Farmer, A. and Brockington,]. (1985). A comparison of clinical and Diagnostic Interview Schedule diagnoses Wfilfl Hrubec, Z. and Omenn, G. S. (1981). Evidence of genetic predisposition to alcoholic 5131713“; and psychosis. Alcoholism; Clinical ma Experimental Research. 5. l Jellinek, E. M. (1952). Current ngt’eBS :61;th of alcohol addiction. W W - 4 91 Joreskog. K and Sorbom.D (1978) W Chicago: National Educational Resources. Kaij,L. and McNeil, T. F. (1960). Genetic aspects of alcoholism. WW Mania} 54-65 Kaprio, J. Koskenvuo, M., Langinvainio, H. Romanov, K. Sarna, S. and Rose, R. (1987). Genetic influences on use and abuse of alcohol. A study of 5638 adult Fitmish twin bmthers WW4]. 349- 355. Knop, J., Angelo, H. and Christensen, J. (1980). Is the role of acetaldehyde 1n alcoholism based on an analytical artifact? m1“) Loehlin, J. C. and Nichols, R. C. (1976). W. Austin: University of Texas Press. Lumeng,].., Hawkins, T. D. andLi, T. K. (1977). InR. G. Thurman (Ed.)_A1:ghglism WWW New York: Academic Press Matheny, A. P. (1979). Appraisal of parental bias 1n twin studies: ascribed zygosity and IQ differences m twins AmfimaticaaMedicautfiemllOoziaamomaLli. 155- 160. McCleam, G. E. and Kakihana, R. (1981). Selective breeding for alcohol sensitivity: Short sleep and long sleep mice. In G. E. McCleam and V. G. Erwin (Eds) .V 0-11 -_.o .111 no.0 2.. ’-..-.1m 0'1 H '.11u01turn £6, Washington, D. C. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. McCord, W. and McCord, J. (1962). A lon tudinal study of the personality of alcoholics. In D. J. Pittman and C. R. nyder (Eds) 3 W New York: I. Wiley and Sons. 1 Mueller, C. and Parcel, T. (1981). Measures of socioeconomic status: alternatives and recommendations W13” Mulford. H. A. and Miller, D. E. (1960). Drinking 1n Iowa V: Drinking and alcoholic dfinldng WWW 483-499 Murray, R. M. Clifford, C. A and Gurling, H. M. (1983). Twin and adoption studies. How good rs the evidence for a genetic role? In M. Galanter (Ed) Rm W 'mAlthOistrzLL New York: Plenum Press. 92 O'Malley, S. S, Carey, K. B. and Maisto, S. A. (1986). Validity of young adult's regard of Parental dn'nking Pracdces lournalOinmAithOismAl. 433- Oreland, L. Witberg, A. Winblad, B. Fowler, C. Gottsfries, C. and Kiianmaa, K. (1983). The activity of monoamine oxidase- A and -B 1n brains from chronic alcoholics WM. 73- 83 Pandey, G. N., Fawcett, J., Gibbons, R., Clark, D. C. and Davis, J. M. (1988). Platelet monoamine oxidase 1n alcoholism. WM 15- 24. Partanen, J., Brunn, K. and Markannen, T. (1966). MW Helsinski: Finnish Foundation for Alcohol Studies. Petrakis, P. L. (1985). Wm. Washington, D. C.: US. Government Printing Office. Petrie. A. (1967). indiudnalimiraflaiaaniiufiefina. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press. Peele, S. (1986). The implications and limitations of genetic models of alcoholism and Other addiCtions IOnnaLotimrhaaonAlthJl. 63- 73 Pollock, V. E., Volavka, J. Mednick, 8., Goodwin, D., Knop, J. and Schulsinger, F. (1984). Electroencephalographic findings. In D. W. Goodwin, K. T. Van Dusen and S A Mednick (Eds )lanarmdmaifiesearchmflmhohsm. Boston: Kluwer- Nijhoff. Propping, P. (1977). Genetic control of ethanol action on the central nervous system. An EEG study 1n twins. W 309- 334. Propping, P. L., Kruger, J. and Nark, M. (1981). Genetic predisposition to alcoholism: an EEG study 1n alcoholics and their relatives. W 51- 58. Puchall, L. B. Coursey, R. D. Buchsbaum, M. and Murphy, D. (1980). Parents of high risk subjects defined levels of monoamine oxidase aetivity. W Miami. 338- 345 Reider, B., Zucker, R. Maguin,. B., Noll, R., and Fitzgerald, H. 0(1989, August). a 010 'vlv 11. --1.1 10 1.01 (tr-10 1011-1101 1-1. 11 ' Paper presented at the 97th annual meeting of the American Psychological Associ: ninn, New Orleans, LA. 93 Roe, A. and Burks, B. (1945). Adult adjusunent of foster children of alcoholic and psychotic parentage and the influence of the foster home. W W. New Haven: Yale University Press. Robins, L. Helzer, J, Croughan, J. and Ratcliffe, K. S. (1980). .St Louis, MO:, Washington University School of Medicine. Scarr, 8. Environmental bias 1n twin studies. (1986). In S. G. Vandenberg (EcL) v Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Scarr, S. and Carter- Saltzmann, L. (1979). Twin method: Defense of a critical assumption W521 542 Schuckit, M. A. (1980). Alcoholigsmztd genetics: Possible biological mediators. WM 4 7- 7 Schuckit, M. A. (1984). Subjective responses to alcohol 1n sons of alcoholics and control subjects WWW 879- 884 Schuckit, M. A. and Rayses, V. (1978). Ethanol ingestion: Differences 1n blood acetaldehyde concentrations in relatives of alcoholics and controls. W, 54- 55. Searles, J. S. (1988). The role of g5e3netics in the pathogenesis of alcoholism. ImmaLQf manual. 1 -167. Selzer, R. (1975). A self-administerflzsgrort Michigan Alcohol Screening Test. Journal OimdrcLQnAlmhM 1 Stevens, G. and Featherman, D. L. (1981). A revised socioeconomic index of OCCUPational Status WW. 364-395 Tarter, R. E., Alterman, A. I. and Edwards, K. L. (1985). Vulnerability to alcoholism 1n 13:11:11. A behavioral-genetic pctscpctive WWW 339- 56 Thompson, W. D. Orvaschel, H. Prusoff, B., and Kidd, K. (1982). An evaluation of the family history methocslgogsascertaining psychiatric disorders. mm W 94 Vaillant. G. E. (1983). WWW Cambl'idgci Harvard University Press. Vaillant, G. E. (1989). The pendulum swings the other way: The role of environment obscured by genes W36. 1151 Von Knoning, A- L. Bohman, M. Von-Knorring, L. and Oreland, L. (1985). Platelet monoamine activity as a biological marker 1n subgroups of alcoholics ._Aota WW2. 51 ~58 Waller, M. B, Mc Bride, W. J., Gatto, G., Lumeng,L, andLi., T. K. (1984). Intragastric self-infusion of ethanol by ethanol preferring and non-prefening rats. W78 80 Winokur, G., Reich T., Rimmer, J. and Pitts, F. N. (1970). Alcoholism III. Diagnosis and familial syclarztric illness 1n 259 alcoholic probands. W Esyohiagy, £3, 1 111. Wittchen, H. Burke, J., Semier, G, Pfister, H., VonCranach, M. and Zaudig, M. (1989). Recall and datings of psychiatric symptoms: Test-retest reliability of time- related symptom questions in a star13dardized psychiatric mterview. Anni-yo: WM 437-44 Zucker, R. A. (1987a). The four alcoholisms: A developmental account of the etiologic process. In P. C. Rivers (Ed) W. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Zucker. R. A. (1989). W Unpublished manuscript. Michigan State University, Department of Psychology, East Lansing, MI. Zucker, R. A. and Davies, W. H. (1989). ’ ' Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, Department of Psychology, East Lansing, MI. Zucker, R. A. and Gomberg, E. A. (1986). Etiology of alcoholism reconsidered. The case for a biopsychosocial process. WOW, 783-793. Zucker, R. A. and Noll, R. B.. (l980b)._l211°[$lng_ond_l2mgfllm Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, Department of Psychology, East Lansing, MI. Zucker, R. A. , Noll, R. B. and Fitzgerald, H. (1986). ' ' ' alsoholios, Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, Demrtment of Psychology, East Lansing, MI. "11111111111111E5