)Al‘. 1.. (’11:! Al AHA-honahhhflmwdt fit. or”, III at .1le fih)...‘ ”I! .141...2.v.1?11...i.v.s.w. ,. ......n.w.- AHHHIEEEQH» 1 1131.- ‘ll , fié‘hflélg ‘ laugh-r1 Ll Eyfitlzilfiifi. 9.19. ‘5 ptlfil I 551‘ n 5‘08 I) I .1“.- EMU/.WWWWIDNMWWW‘I. (v $41. L Mme-mimw... i. . . . . : : .3 . .2... Emu . w. .. . $.35... . .r, a .w «a. a... , .. _.-.4. w... a"... . ...v$....«..¢ wwwmwrwwn h.- a a. . . . ) «3.5“; 0... w. . . . .4.~4..u 5- 5.5 WWV - Ins . ., . _ . ., wflwmmufi... k:. .x m .3: WWW-v” . - .. ; twa- AX .fi. I. . . _ P . a». (J .m D. . a. ,- _ .J 9.1.. 3.5.3” ., ., . . . . .:i It 5%; . ” $5.3" .. .59.“...5? 3+. . . . . ,.. . . , . . . ‘ 7 _ 7 / .111: rhz .14.”? .‘u ‘4... n .14).. Kill-1.2.... . mg : ._....H-._. __.. 2...; .35..” .. . 3;: : ...:.:.... ...:.::..: :1: . f. ,3» 1-... S .12.. . 7.... 3-; 1.3.1.7.. ,1 - E. .1... 2.: .. z: : : :5.- .; : @- MICHIGAN Hllllllllllllll . \l 31 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Factors Influencing EAP Utilization: A Control Theory Perspective presented by Suzanne Marie Crampton has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for l l Ph.D.(kgmfin Business Administratiod l i l l ', /.7 / g , 7x / (Niajorprofes/sé Date 12/4/92 1 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LiBRARV Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution c:\clrc\datedue.pm3-o. ' FACTORS INFLUENCING EAP UTILIZATION: A CONTROL THEORY PERSPECTIVE By Suzanne Marie Crampton A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Management 1 992 iIi IIII ABSTRACT FACTORS INFLUENCING EAP UTILIZATION: A CONTROL THEORY PERSPECTIVE By Suzanne Marie Crampton The purpose of the present research was to examine factors which may influence an individual’s decision to utilize services at an Employee Assistance Program (EAP). Past research that explored EAP usage typically examined demographic characteristics of EAP users and the types of problems presented at the EAP in order to create a profile of EAP users. The major problem with this research is that organizations gained little practical knowledge because it did not help us understand why individuals decide to use/not use an EAP. In addition, there was little theoretical basis for including research variables. The present research incorporated into this decision process individual personality variables (Health Locus of Control, or HLOC), beliefs held by individuals regarding health-related issues (health goals, social group health norms, past and present health status) and perceptions regarding an EAP (expectations regarding its ability to improve one’s health and perceived support, pressure, and barriers to using an BAP). A Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization was developed based on control theory which assumes that behavior is goal directed, and this model was used to provide a structure to studying factors hypothesized to influence the EAP decision process. A questionnaire was mailed to a sample consisting of both EAP users and non— users who were employed by a large organization and who had equal access to the on— site BAP. Seven hypotheses were examined using multiple regression analyses. Three factors were predicted to influence an individual’s desired health goal level, but only one hypothesis was partially supported when Chance HLOC was found to have a negative relationship to one’s desired goal level. Of the variables hypothesized to influence the EAP utilization decision, Internal HLOC and Chance HLOC were found to moderate the relationship between a perceived discrepancy in one’s current and desired health and the decision to utilize EAP services. In addition, perceptions of both personal and work-related sources of support, pressure, and barriers to using an EAP were found to significantly impact an individual’s decision to seek EAP assistance. Implications of these results, limitations, and recommendations for future research are discussed. To my best friend and husband, Dave, without whose years of love, support and encouragement this would not have been possible and to my parents, Eugene and Mary Churchill, with thanks for their love, prayers, and support throughout all my educational experiences. This is dedicated to each of them as a way to express all they have done for me. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The easiest task of this research is to thank those individuals most responsible for assisting me in the completion of this project. I would like to thank my dissertation chair, Dr. Daniel Ilgen, for the many hours he spent providing guidance and support. His efforts have been invaluable and contributed significantly to not only the research methodology but to the overall quality of this dissertation. His valuable ideas and refinements to the document helped me deveIOp professionally. I wish to also express my appreciation to my other committee members, Dr. Alison E. Barber and Dr. Richard Block, for their timely comments and assistance which contributed to the completion of the final product. I also appreciate the assistance received from the director of MSU’s Employee Assistance Program, Thomas Helma, who helped me obtain the necessary approval and access to the research site and sample. The completion of my doctoral program was facilitated by my colleagues and the faculty and staff in the Department of Management. They were instrumental in my professional development and their efforts are also reflected in this research. I would also like to express my appreciation for the financial assistance provided toward this project by The International Foundation of Employee Benefit Plans for their research grant which assisted in the completion of the study. I am also very appreciative for being awarded the Harvey Wilson/SHRM Foundation Doctoral Dissertation Research Grant to aid in completing this project. Finally, I wish to express my gratitude to my husband, my parents, my three stepchildren and the rest of my family who had to put up with the very limited amounts of time available while I completed the doctoral program. Their unrelenting love and support made my accomplishments possible and worthwhile. vi Will TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................. x LIST OF FIGURES ............................................... x1 CHAPTER 1 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM .................................... 1 Introduction ...................................................... 1 Employee Assistance Programs ........................................ 5 The Pervasiveness of the Need for EAPs ........................... 11 The Costs Of Troubled Employees ................................ 14 The Benefits of EAPS .......................................... 17 Diversity of EAPs ............................................ :2 Assumptions of the Research ........................................ Organization of the Research ........................................ 26 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF EAP AND HEALTH-RELATED LITERATURE ............. 28 Historical Background .............................................. 3: Historical Background to 1940--Welfare Movement ................... 33 Occupational Alcoholism Programs (OAPs) ........................ The 1970’s to the Present-—The Legal Influences on EAPs .............. 38 The 1970’s to the Present--Additional Influences on EAPs ............. :3 The Wellness Concept ......................................... 43 Summary of Models/Attitudes Regarding Employee Health ................. 48 The Structure and Diversity of EAPs .................................. 57 EAP Research .................................................... 59 Research on EAP Utilization/Participation ......................... 61 MaritalStatus ()2 Gender ................................................ 62 Race 63 Age ................................................... 63 Tenure/Seniority ....................................... ()4 OccupationalLevel.....................................:. 65 Income ............................................... 66 Education ....................... . . .. .................. ()6 Problem Type and EAP Utilization/PartICIpatIon ................ vii CHAPTER 2 (cont’d) Referral Source and EAP Utilization/Participation ............... 67 Summary ........................................................ 72 CHAPTER 3 CONTROL THEORY AND A MODEL OF EAP UTILIZATION ........... 76 Control Theory ................................................... 76 A Control Theory Perspective on Health-Related Activities ................. 91 A Systems Perspective on EAPs ...................................... 94 A Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization ............................ 96 Overview of the Model ........................................ 96 The Goal/Standard ........................................... 102 Health History ......................................... 107 Hypothesis #1 .................................... 108 Health Locus Of Control .................................. 108 Hypothesis #2 .................................... 112 Social Group Health Values/Norms ......................... 113 Hypothesis #3 .................................... 115 Summary Of the Health Goal Component ..................... 116 The Sensor/Feedback Function ................................. 116 The Comparator Mechanism ................................... 117 The Behavior/Effector Component ............................... 119 Health Locus of Control .................................. 120 Hypothesis #4a .................................... 122 Hypothesis #4b ................................... 126 Hypothesis #4c .................................... 126 Expectancy ............................................ 126 Hypothesis #5 .................................... 129 Support/Pressure/Barriers ................................. 129 Personal Sources of SupportJPressure and Barriers ......... 133 Hypothesis #6 ................................ 136 Work-Related Sources of Support/Pressure and Barriers ..... 136 Hypothesis #7 ................................ 143 Summary ....................................................... 143 CHAPTER 4 METHODOLOGY ............................................... 146 Power Analysis .................................................. 146 Research Site ................................................... 147 Sample ........................................................ 149 Human Rights ................................................... 159 Study Design/Procedures ........................................... 159 Pilot Tests ...................................................... 160 Measures ....................................................... 162 Health History .............................................. 162 viii CHAPTER 4 (cont’d) Health Locus of Control ...................................... 164 Social Group Health Values/Norms .............................. 166 Desired Health Goal Level ..................................... 167 Current Health Status ........................................ 168 Health Goal-Health Status Discrepancy ........................... 169 Expectancy Of Goal Attainment ................................. 170 Support/Pressure/Barriers to Using the EAP ....................... 171 EAP Utilization ............................................. 174 Other Variables ............................................. 174 Analytic Methods ................................................ 174 CHAPTER 5 RESULTS ...................................................... 177 Hypothesis #1 .................................................. 181 Hypothesis #2 .................................................. 182 Hypothesis #3 .................................................. 182 Hypothesis #4a, #4b, and #4c ..................................... 184 Hypothesis #5 .................................................. 189. Hypothesis #6 .................................................. 189 Hypothesis #7 .................................................. 192 Post-Hoe Analyses ................................................ 192 CHAPTER 6 DISCUSSION ................................................... I97 Hypothesis #1: Effect of Health History on Desired Health Goal ............ 197 Hypothesis #2: Effect of Health Locus Of Control on Desired Health Goal . . . . 201 Hypothesis #3: Effect Of Social Group Health Values/Norms on Desired Health Goal ......................................... 202 Summary of Relationships With Desired Health Goal ..................... 205 Hi’Pothesis #4a, #4b and #4c: The Moderator Influence Of Health Locus of Control ............................................ 208 HYpothesis #5: The Moderator Influence of Expectancy ...... . ............ 213 Hypothesis #6 and # 7: Effect of Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers . . and Work Support/Pressure/Barriers on EAP Utilization .............. 217 leltations and Future Research .................................... 221 Summary ....................................................... 226 LIST OF REFERENCES .......................................... 228 APPENDICES Appendix A - Definition of Terms ................................... 32; Appendix B - Survey Instrument ..................................... ix LIST OF TABLES Table 1: EAP Cost—Impact Studies .................................. 20 Table 2: Descriptive Statistics of Sample ............................ 152 Table 3: Descriptive Statistics of Sample (Means of Demographics) ........ 154 Table 4: Frequencies of Demographic Variables by EAP Usage/Non-Usage . . 157 Table 5: Descriptive Statistics of Major Variables in the Model ........... 163 Table 6: Means and Standard Deviations of Major Variables in the Model . . 178 Table 7: Intercorrelations Among Major Variables in the Model .......... 179 Table 8: Regression Results for the Health Locus Of Control (HLOC) Subscales on Desired Health Goal Level ...................... 183 Table 9: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Health Locus of . Control Subscales, Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction ..... 185 Table 10: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Expectancy, Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction ................... 190 Table 11: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Demographic Variables, Support/Pressure/Barriers Variables, and Personal Beliefs/Value Variables .............................................. 194 Table 12: Crosstabulation Analysis of Availability of Other Resources and EAP Utilization ..................................... 196 Figure 1: Figure 2: Figure 3: Figure 4: Figure 5: Figure 6: Figure 7: Figure 8: Figure 9: Figure 10: Figure 11: Figure 12a: Figure 12b: Figure 12c: Figure 13: Figure 14: Figure 15: LIST OF FIGURES Approaches To Health At Work ............................. Hierarchy of Man and Science .............................. Flow Diagram of Key EAP Activities ......................... The TOTE Unit ......................................... The Negative Feedback Loop ............................... Cybernetic Model of Self-Attention Processes .................. Control Systems Model of Motivation ......................... An Integrated Control Theory Model of Work Motivation ......... Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization .................... Factors Affecting Desired Health Goal Level .................. Relationship Between Perceived Discrepancy and EAP Utilization Hypothesized Relationship of Internal Health Locus of Control . . . . Hypothesized Relationship of Powerful Others Health Locus of Control ................................................ Hypothesized Relationship of Chance Health Locus of Control . . . . Hypothesized Relationship of Expectancy ..................... Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Internal Health Locus of Control (IHLC) ........ Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (PHLC) xi 44 47 49 8O 86 90 100 103 104 123 124 125 130 186 187 LIST OF FIGURES (cont’d) Figure 16: Results of Regressing EAP UtilizatiOn on Perceived Discrepancy by Chance Health Locus of Control (Cl-ILC) ........ 188 Figure 17: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Expectancy ................................ 191 xii CHAPTER 1 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Introduction The traditional barrier that has existed between an employee’s personal problems and an employer’s involvement in the personal lives of employees has been breaking down during the past two decades as more and more employers have extended help to employees through the establishment of employee assistance programs, or EAPs. Employees often experience personal problems, such as alcoholism, drug abuse, social and psychological problems that negatively impact their work performance. Organizations have found these problems can manifest themselves in the form of excessive absenteeism, tardiness, industrial accidents, insurance claims, and grievances along with decreases in efficiency, employee health and morale-which, in turn, affect organizations’ overall profit, quality and efficiency. Thus, maintaining a healthy workforce will result in economic benefits for the employee, the firm, and for society. Health is currently defined as ”a state of complete physical,-rnental, and social well- being" (Stone, 1979). Health behavior is concerned with factors influencing one’s choice among alternative methods of dealing with bodily threats (Stone, 1979). Health is viewed not only as the absence of disease but as both an ideal "state" to be achieved as well as a "motive" where one strives to develop and adapt behaviors to respond more appropriately to the environment (Dubos, 1965; Stone, 1979). Experts distinguish between health and illness behaviors where health behavior refers to activity undertaken by a healthy individual to prevent disease while illness behavior is activity undertaken by someone who feels ill in order to Obtain relief (Kasl & Cobb, 1966). 2 In 1980, a-national study found that the annual economic cost Of alcohol, drug, and mental disorders was $190.6 billionuwith over $75 billion of the costs related to reduced productivity at work (DHHS, 1982). In 1987, health costs were $400-500 billion, or 10% of the nation’s GNP (Bureau of the Census, 1986; Terborg, 1986). It has been estimated that these expenditures will exceed $1 trillion before the year 2000 (Wriston, 1982). Employers typically pay 27-30% Of these costs (Terborg, 1986). Good employee health is an industrial policy that helps to serve the selfointerests of industry. Melvin Glasser of the United Auto Workers stated in 1969 that the workplace should be used as a focal point for providing health service for job-related diseases as well as preventive health services, health education, and all health problems. Business and labor organizations have endorsed interventions to enhance employee health and contain health costs—-one of which is the employee assistance program (EAP). Employee Assistance Programs are defined as: Policies, procedures and services which respond to employees whosepeisonal, emotional or behavioral problems interfere directly or indirectly with work performance by providing confidential counseling and/or professional information, care or referral to appropriate sources for help (Schmitz, 1982, p.3). The term EAP is a generic term for all occupational programs that enable troubled employees (i.e., those whose work history is characterized by productivity problems, absenteeism, accidents and other job-related problems) to receive help for a variety of problems-~ranging from alcohol and drug abuse, gambling addiction and eating disorders to child-care and pre- and post-retirement counseling—by either identifying or responding to employees whose problems interfere with their work performance (Bureau of National Affairs, 1987; Leavitt, 1983; McCroskey, 1982; Thomas, 1982). Diverse 3 educational, physical fitness and recreational programs may also be included in such programs (Thomas, 1982). A more extensive review of the relevant literature will be presented in the second chapter. Based on the review in that chapter, the following conclusions will be drawn. First, despite the diversity and scope of EAPs today, and the fact that a significant number of employees may be in need of assistance, there are many employees reluctant to utilize the services when needed. Second, to date most research has been atheoretical in nature. There appears to be an absence of theory surrounding the relationships influencing health and help-seeking behaviors. Third, several variables have been neglected surrounding EAP research. For example, McKinlay (1975) argued that "organizational phenomena may be as highly related to utilization behavior as the personal characteristics of users" (p. 257). It is concluded that there is a limited theoretical basis guiding the study of organizational assistance programs. While there have been conceptual models of the EAP referral process developed by a number of researchers (e.g., Savoca, 1986; Wrich, 1980b), 3 specific model outlining factors which influence an individual’s decision to seek assistance from an EAP has not been developed. A number of variables have been examined to gain knowledge on what a typical profile of an EAP user resembles. However, most of the data gathered to date have focused on factors that can be obtained directly from EAP records-gender, race, age, job status, income, problem type, etc. These factors offer little understanding of why some employees utilize EAP services while others do not. Furthermore, many of the conclusions reached on EAP participation factors have often been inconclusive or contradictory. 4 The purposes of the present research are (1) to examine factors which might influence an employee’s decision to seek EAP assistance and (2) to develop a conceptual model to help guide future research on employee usage of EAP services. A model of the utilization process will be developed based on a control theory perspective. Control theory has been applied to behavioral issues in many areas of study, including the areas of cognitive, clinical, and health psychology (Carver & Scheier, 1981, 1982; Wiener, 1948). Control theory is based on the key concept of a standard or reference value and a process whereby sensed information, or feedback, is compared to this standard or goal (Carver & Scheier, 1981; Miller et al., 1960; Powers, 1973; Von Bertalanffy, 1968; Wiener, 1948). If a discrepancy between these variables exists, a force is created to motivate an individual to reduce the standard-feedback discrepancy via affective, behavioral, and/or cognitive responses (Taylor, Fisher & Ilgen, 1984). Because "good health" is considered a standard and since many factors influencing the development, amelioration, or prevention of disease and illness are considered under the control of an individual, control theory is seen as an acceptable model to use as a guideline in examining health-related behaviors (Carver & Scheier, 1982; Karoly, 1985). Specific health activities where control theory has provided a useful framework are behaviors related to monitoring one’s current health state (e.g., examining one’s pulse, blood pressure, or temperature) to determine if one’s health is less than ideal and, if not, motivating the individual to action (Carver & Scheier, 1982; Leventhal, Meyer & Nerenz, 1980; Schwartz, 1978, 1979). The present study attempts to examine relationships among variables of interest in health and help-seeking behaviors--specifically, variables influencing an employee’s Ila oil Ita 5 seeking assistance at an EAP. Ilgen (1990) suggests that the timing, frequency and nature of access to health care services by those in need have important consequences to employees (e.g., health, quality of life) and organizations (e.g., financial, productivity); however, these health care system access behaviors are not well understood. The aim of the present study is a scientific one by contributing to our knowledge of help—seeking behaviors in an EAP setting and to the development of a theoretical basis for examining the relationships involved. The aim is also a practical one by enabling organizations, human resource managers, and EAP staff members to better understand the utilization process. In the following section, an overview of EAPs will be presented. Employee Assistance Programs Employee Assistance Programs exist within a variety of organizational environments-business, sports, government, hospitals, public utilities, entertainment, educational, and labor unions (Castro, 1986). Employers generally agree that personal problems are still a private domain, except when such problems have a negative impact on work behavior. More and more these problems are being addressed at work through EAPs, with the expectation that the benefits will be greater than the costs incurred in establishing such programs;e.g., decreases in absenteeism and in the costs of hiring and training replacements, and increases in morale and productivity (Lyons, 1987, p. 38). Many forces began to evolve that changed both the attitude of the employee as well as the employer since the 1960’s. For example, American workers have been in a state of transition during the last couple of decades. The transitions have occurred for many reasons, which include: the median educational level for employees has increased from 8.7 years in 1940 to 12.7 years by 1984; 44% of the workforce is made up of women; only ill III 6 one-third of the households in the US. have only one spouse employed; 52% of female employees have children less than six years of age; and increased automation and technology have resulted in increased specialization of workers and training costs (Hayghe, 1984; McClellan, 1985). These transitions have, in turn, brought about changes in employee attitudes and expectations whereby workers expect more from work than just a job and a paycheck. Employees demand a healthy work environment, and good jobs are assessed based on a variety of social and psychological needs. ”Workers are no longer content to be just economic tools in the production of goods and services. They want to be treated as human beings who have hopes, aspirations, anxiety, and fears that need to be recognized" (Ozawa, 1980, p. 466). Societal changes have also taken place during the last several years. The number of alcoholics, drug abusers, divorced couples, single parents, step-parents, and those caring for elderly relatives has increased (Masi, 1984; Myers, 1984). These changes, along with the higher educational levels of employees, increasing numbers of women and minorities in the labor force, high requirements of technical competence, changing management practices, and foreign competition all impact on the workplace. As society and workforce attitudes changed over the years, a humanization of the workplace began to emerge (Cascio, 1986). As we begin the 1990’s, business realizes the importance of human resources to successful operations, and nearly all believe that the most productive workers are healthy-mot only physically but also mentally. The effects of increasing threats of foreign competition have further compelled the business world to modify its practices and programs to meet the challenges faced. ill 7 While the above changes in society and the workforce were occurring, organizations began recognizing the need to take a more holistic approach when dealing with their employees. In a 1974 survey of management personnel by the American Management Association, 90% thought corporations should be concerned with the total person and not just with daily output (Work, 1974). Based on his research for the National Institute of Business and Industrial Chaplains, Brown (1983) identified the following as major industrial concerns: marriage and family, alcohol abuse, pastoral care, crisis situations, other personal concerns (e.g., anxiety, depression), job-related problems, financial problems, mental illness, drug abuse, and court/legal issues (pp. 14-15). Employers have also been forced to pay more attention to the interpersonal and personal concerns of employees due to the legal constraints and requirements placed on organizations. Examples of such federal and state legislation include Title IX and Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, as amended, Title VI (apprenticeships), and Executive Order 11246, as amended (sex discrimination) (Bloom, 1986). These acts mandate that organizations avoid discrimination based on non-work related factors and take affirmative action to increase job opportunities of members of protected classes. As a result of these forces, firms developed human resource programs to address the needs and welfare of employees and assist workers in dealing with both personal and work-related problems; i.e., programs such as participative management, expanded benefits, improved organizational structure, and employee counseling services (Carr & Hellan, 1980; Shamir & Bargall, 1982). While these all improved organizational life, they did not address the needs of the employee as well as the EAP has (Carr & Hellan, 1980). Management and unions both recognized that EAPs were effective for meeting both competitive challenges and employee needs. The work site was also viewed as an 8 effective location for the delivery Of health programs since the workplace is where employees spend one-third or more Of their time (Alderman, 1984; Fielding, 1984a). Terborg (1988) cited three additional reasons why the work site is an excellent place to conduct health promotion activities: (1) a large number of people are employed on a regular basis, (2) there is a potential for manipulating the social and physical environments, and (3) there is the possibility of reducing health-care costs and increasing productivity. Other forces that make the work site an effective EAP environment and move troubled employees to seek EAP assistance include the fact that serious personal problems often impair work performance in some manner, organizations expect employees to maintain certain standards of performance, there is a strong desire on the part of an employee to keep his/her job, the employee Often is under pressure from external sources (e.g., coworkers, spouse, friends) to get some help, and the fact that help is often available and easily accessible in the form of an EAP (Masi, 1984; Myers, 1984). One question that might still be asked is whether the employer’s involvement in an EAP is an unwarranted encroachment into the personal lives of employees. The answer is ”no" for several reasons. Most employees, for instance, keep their personal problems from affecting their job performance because income is important to them, so employers only become involved when the problem is beyond what the individual can handle and deteriorating job performance is observed. Also, many employees do not know where to receive help for problems or can not afford help from external resources, so EAPs at the workplace are useful for assisting employees. For example, Pardue (1987) found that if the company had not established an EAP, of the 200 clients interviewed, only 25% stated they would have sought assistance for their problem at an external resource. 9 Finally, employers do not become directly involved in the specific personal problems Of an employee since confidentiality is a requirement for any EAP to succeed and survive. Initial efforts on the part Of employers to assist employees were in the area of alcohol-related problems. Threatt (1976) noted that researchers have accumulated a substantial amount of evidence to support the assumption that the use of alcohol causes alterations in human physical and cognitive functioning. Consequently, the first occupational programs were typically low-keyed and designed along the lines Of Alcoholics Anonymous (Roman, 1983a). The EAPs of today evolved out of these original alcoholism programs. Major federal initiatives undertaken in 1972 resulted in more formal Occupational Alcoholism Programs (OAPs) (Masi, 1984). The early OAPs functioned mostly on a trial and error basis because of the lack of experience with such programs. Since society during the 1940’s and 1950’s viewed an employee with an alcohol problem as weak or immoral, program effectiveness was hampered as companies took a more punitive approach to dealing with employees by threatening or dismissing them. However, by the 1960’s OAPs were reported to save money and production time (Trice & Schonbrunn, 1981). It was this success that led to the realization that help might be successfully provided for other employee problems, which further lead to the development of EAPs (Trice & Schonbrunn, 1981). Since the 1960’s, EAPs have changed in form, grown in numbers, and gained in popularity. By 1980, Wrich estimated that EAPs had been adopted by approximately 60% of the Fortune 500 companies and thousands of others (Wrich, 1980b). There are now over 10,000 EAPs operating in the US. covering millions Of workers (BNA, 1987; Champion, 1988). Five significant factors have influenced this growth. First, the 10 enactment of the Hughes Act, or the Comprehensive Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Prevention, Treatment and Rehabilitation Act, in 1970 provided federal funding for state programs and created’the National Institute of Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse in 1971 (Forrest, 1983; Masi & Teems, 1983). Second, the Secretary of Health and Human Services was authorized to promote the development of “screening, consultation, referral and education programs at employment sites to detect and prevent early mental health problems" (Mental Health Systems Act Of 1980, Section 208). This made the EAP concept a public policy issue and corporations became interested as a matter of "social responsibility" (Scanlon, 1986). Third, the passage of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973 prohibited discrimination against any handicapped employee and required employers to provide services for troubled employees before terminating them. In addition, "handicapped" was interpreted to include those suffering from alcohol, drug, mental or emotional problems (Masi & T eems, 1983; Roman, 1981). Fourth, the US. labor force experienced its first drop in the productivity level for hourly employees and employers became concerned that the drop may be due partly to employees experiencing personal or work-related problems (Shuster, 1978). A final factor was that organizations were faced with increased foreign competition and needed to develop new ways to improve profit (Forrest, 1983; Googins, 1975). Employee assistance programs today cover a broad array of personal problems and, thus, are able to reach a larger population than the more narrowly-focused alcohol programs. They have also demonstrated their positive impact on health costs, productivity, and job performance (Masi, 1984; Myers, 1984; Wrich, 1980b). 11 The Pervasiveness of the Need for EAPs While some employees can successfully separate their work from their personal life and continue to function well on the job, others bring their problems to work, resulting in a variety of dysfunctional behaviors (e.g., tardiness, poor performance, poor inter- relationships with supervisors and coworkers, increased accidents and grievances, etc.). Experts differ in their perception of the pervasiveness of ”troubled" employees. Storm (1977) has pointed out that in any given employee population, at least ten percent‘will be seriously troubled by personal problems, which include medical, alcohol and drug related, emotional, marital, family, financial, or other problems. However, others suggest this figure is much higher (Baxter, 1981; Cahill, 1983; Egdahl & Walsh, 1980; Weiner et al., 1973; Wrich, 1980). There are over 110 million drinkers in America with an estimated 10.2 million adult problem drinkers, an additional 3 million problem drinkers ranging from age 14 to 17, and 100,000 to 200,000 new cases of alcoholism annually (New York Executive Chamber, 1982; NIAAA, 1978). In a 1985 Gallup poll, one-third of all families reported substance abuse in a family member (BN A, 1987). Baxter (1981) estimated that alcohol problems affect between 5-15% of the workforce and Applebaum (1982) cited estimates that alcoholics cut across all organizational levels-25% whiteicollar, 30% manual workers, and 45% professional and managerial. Others have estimated that another 3 to 5% of the workforce is involved in drug abuse (Baxter, 1981) and that as many as 20 to 30% of the US. working population has a serious personal problem (Cahill, 1983; Egdahl & Walsh, 1980; Weiner et al., 1973; Wrich, 1980b). In a survey of top executives, however, Roman (1978) found that most failed to perceive the problems as severe as those researchers cited in the preceding paragraph, 12 although they agreed that no organization is exempt from these problems. Trice and Roman (1972) estimated that alcohol and drug abuse problems in the workplace affect between 3 and 4% of the workforce, and the frequency of other employee problems is another 3 or 4%. While experts may disagree as to the size of the problem, no one can question whether a problem exists. For instance, according to Trice and Roman (1972): When the potential impact of any one deviant drinker is considered, . . . the relevance of the problem for organizational functioning mounts rapidly. In other words, the disruptive consequences of deviant drinking may far exceed the cost entailed if 4% of the work force were absent or simply sat at their jobs and did practically nothing. The very essence of a work organization is the inter- dependence of job performances. Deviance by one employee may ”reverberate” beyond his work station or desk, sometimes disrupting an entire organization. Thus the prevalence figures alone do not tell the full story. (p. 2) Thus, it seems a snowball effect is produced as a result of any one employee’s dysfunctional work behavior in the form of direct costs of the worker (absenteeism, poor productivity) and indirect costs as work group activities and morale are disrupted and supervisors must spend time and attention dealing with deviant behaviors. While employees are affected by non—work related problems, the unique characteristics of the work setting have also been recognized as influencing the emotional health of employees. For example, much has been written about stress and burnout at work. Industrial stress has been estimated to account for $32 billion annually in work- related accidents, and contributes to heart disease-which is responsible for losses of over 135 million work days annually (McClellan, 1985, pp. 29-30). Vicary and Resnik (1982) reported that job-related stress is one of the most frequent reasons for drug abuse and other health problems. Examples of job stress include: physical environment factors (e.g., temperature, illumination, noise, office design) (Quick & Quick, 1984), organizational stressors (e.g., deadline pressures, failure to obtain promotions, fear of failure, job insecurity, l3 competition, task demands, long working hours, hazardous conditions, organizational tolerance for deviant behavior) (Brodsky, 1977; Landy & Trumbo, 1976; Martin & Schermerhorn, 1983) and individual level workplace stressors (e.g., personality clashes, social density, group pressure, labeling, social support) (House, 1981; Levinson, 1976). Yankelovich (1978) adds there is stress resulting from the tension. between the old cultural values where employees become subsumed in their job, which still prevail in the workplace, and the new values of workers today who expect personalized, self—fulfilling work. Trice and Roman (1972) discuss four work environment factors which increase the probability of substance—related deviance: (1) lack of visibility, such as job positions with flexible hours and those which keep the employee isolated from supervisors and coworkers; (2) absence of structure; (3) absence of social controls, such as when drinking is part of the work role; and (4) miscellaneous factors, such as roles which place individuals under severe strain, competitive work climates, and employees who are illegal drug users. House (1974) suggested the following aspects should be investigated regarding work stress: objective work conditions conducive to stress, individual perceptions of stressful work situations, individual responses and outcomes to perceived stress, and individual or social situational characteristics that condition the relationship between the first four factors. Specific employee groups have been found to be under a great deal of stress, such as employees who work on rotating shifts who suffer stress due to irregular sleep patterns, poor nutrition, and other pressures as a result of irregular work patterns (e.g., flight attendants, nurses, police officers, factory workers) (Fever, 1983). Women also are often under great stress from the multiple role expectations and conflicting demands placed on them (Roth, 1981). 14 Role conflict (when an employee receives ambiguous and/or conflicting demands from others at work) and role ambiguity (responses to behavior are unpredictable or role requirements are unclear) are the most heavily researched aspects of job stress (Knapp, 1985). Role ambiguity and conflict have been positively associated with somatic complaints, depression, irritation, anxiety and tension (Caplan et al., 1975; French, Caplan & Van Harrison, 1982; Margolis, Kroes & Quinn (1974). No longer are employees today satisfied with just receiving a wage, and when other needs are unmet, many employees feel frustration and stress. One reason found by researchers for alcohol abuse is that alcohol may be used as a coping strategy against stress (Lazarus, 1974; Pearlin & Radabaugh, 1976; Williams, Calhoun & Ackoff, 1982). However, the effect of stress on health appears to depend on the context of the stressful agent, how individuals perceive it, and the social supports and resources available to the individual (Breznitz & Goldberger, 1982; Cohen & Syme, 1985). Occupational stress management programs typically focus on treatment and helping employees cope with stress through such methods as assertiveness training, biofeedback, and coping skills training, rather than on prevention or removing the sources of stress (Everly, 1984; Ganster, Mayes, Sime & Tharp, 1982; Pelletier & Lutz, 1988). The Costs of Troubled Employees In presenting the costs involved in employing and assisting troubled employees, one type of cost to employers is represented in the many costs associated with the deteriorating work performance Of troubled employees. It has been estimated that 70 million American workers function at only half their daily capacity (United States Congress, 1982). As mentioned in the previous section, for example, a major health 15 problem in the US. is alcohol abuse where over half of the adult problem drinkers are employed across all organizational levels (Applebaum, 1982). These employees cost American business over $42 billion annually (White House Office of Drug Abuse Policy, 1978)--costs which are manifested in a variety of work behaviors of alcoholic employees: excessive absences, on-the-job accidents, and health benefits (Kuzmits & Hammonds, 1979). In addition, Wrich (1980b) estimates a 25% loss of efficiency per alcoholic employee-or $5,000 a year for an employee earning $20,000. Furthermore, alcoholism has been cited as a possible cause in 70% of all filed grievances (Wrich, 1980b). Employees with substance abuse problems average between two and three times more absences, three times more sick leave and accident benefits, and five times more compensation claims than employees without such problems (Pattison & Kaufman, 1983; Wrich, 1980b). Hall (1983) added that 10% of the employed population that abuses alcohol or other drugs produce at 25% below capacity and the average firm has a loss of 2.5% in payroll costs. In addition to the costs of substance-abuse problems described above, it has been estimated that employees’ emotional problems and stresses cost industry billions of dollars in absenteeism and turnover costs, excessive tardiness, negative work attitudes, increased employee alienation from the workplace, decreased American productivity, annual increases in health insurance claims and other costs (Baxter, 1981; Berry & Boland, 1977; Busch, 1981; Carr & Hellan, 1980; Follman, 1978; Masi, 1984; Murray, 1983; Shain & Groeneveld, 1980; Trice & Roman, 1972). For example, it has been reported that business loses from $1,622 to over $3,000 per employee annually due to emotional problems (Myers, 1934)- IOU 16 Experts have found that workers experiencing stress from jobvrelated factors are less efficient, experience reduced concentration, greater absenteeism, decreased morale, have greater problems handlingjob pressures, increased turnover, and increased negative health outcomes (Cooper, 1981; Fly, 1980; House, 1974; Matteson & Ivancevich, 1982). It has also been estimated that 80-90% of all industrial accidents may be traced to personal problems, while emotional problems are implicated in 65-80% of all employee terminations (Brown, 1973; Egdahl & Walsh, 1980; Pati & Adkins, 1983). Smoking is another major health issue that has been reported to reduce mental efficiency by 23% and result in 77 million work days lost per year (Myers, 1984). It has been reported that a person’s psychological makeup is often associated with ulcers, obesity, migraine headaches, arthritis, colitis, and some forms of cancer--costs which organizations experience in increased health costs (Compcare, 1981). In a recent article, it was reported that it was costing companies an average of $1,985 per worker each year in health-care benefits (Employee Health Care, 1988). Others have estimated that costs of lost productivity in American industry range from 829 billion dollars a year, and when drug abuse and other problems are included, the figures increase from 30-70 billion dollars each year (Applebaum, 1982; Berry & Boland, 1977; Egdahl & Walsh, 1980; Follman, 1978; United States Congress, 1982). While the costs of troubled employees have been great to organizations, there are also high personal costs involved for troubled employees and their families. For example, researchers have stated that suicide rates are 58% higher for alcoholics compared to the national average, alcohol is cited as a cause in 55% of all auto accidents, in 64% of all fatal auto accidents, in 40% of all cases brought before family courts, in 11% of all annual deaths, in 50% of home accidents, that from 25 to 50 17 percent of patients in the hospital are suffering from an alcohol-related illness, and that 20% of all referrals to child—guidance clinics are children of alcoholics (BNA, 1987; Kinney & Leaton, 1983; Matsunaga, 1983; McClellan, 1982; United States Congress, 1978). Nonealcoholic family members (in families with an alcoholic) are also a financial burden to business since these individuals have been found to have sick leave costs ten times more than a control group (Compcare, 1981; Wrich, 1986). Substance abuse has also been cited as a contributing factor in 50 to 60 percent of drownings and 80 percent of all suicide cases (Wrich, 1986). There are other costs incurred by the organization which involve the implementation and administration of an EAP. Some of these costs include: (1) the compensation of the EAP staff; (b) office expenses for renting/leasing office space, paying for utilities, furniture, equipment, and supplies; (c) training expenses for renting/buying materials and time spent in training (training of supervisors, employees, and EAP staff); ((1) provision of EAP services; and (e) liability and health benefits insurance (Myers, 1984, p. 109). Various estimates have been cited for these costs. James Wrich’s cost estimate is $67,220 for the first year of operation for a company with 1,000 employees with long—term costs over 25 years estimated at $426,740 (Masi, 1984, p. 198). Westrate (1983) estimated that the average cost per employee at one EAP was $1.50 a month, or $18.00 annually. Others have estimated the cost to be approximately $5.00 per employee annually (Masi, 1984; Schlenger & Hayward, 1975). The Benefits Of EAPs Despite the various costs involved in establishing and maintaining EAPs, many researchers have indicated the benefits outweigh the costs. Several early evaluation Iii SUI] 18 studies conducted on OAP effectiveness (Asma, Eggert & Hilker, 1971; Googins & Kurtz, 1981; Hoffman & Roman, 1984; Kurtz, Googins & Howard, 1984) generally reported positive results. For example, a 1980 cost-benefit study at the Illinois Bell alcohol-related EAP tracked 752 problem drinkers referred to the EAP. The study found that after referral to the EAP, 66% of the employees had "good" job performance ratings compared with 90% having ”fair to poor” ratings prior to referral, and that disability claims decreased 52%, off—duty accidents decreased 42.4%, and on-duty accidents decreased 61.4% after referral (BNA, 1987). Similarly, evaluative studies of broad—brush EAPs have generally reported positive outcomes (County of Alameda, 1978; DuPont, 1979; Foote et al., 1978; Washington Business Group on Health, 1978). Unfortunately, there appears to be little or no consensus on how to measure EAP effectiveness. Most evaluation measures relate to the goals of the particular program. Jerrell and Rightmyer (1982) reviewed 38 empirical EAP studies published from 1958 to 1980 which focused on four types of measures employed in EAP evaluation. The first set covers accidents, sick leave, and medical~ surgical costs. The second group focuses on outcome measures that include absenteeism, tardiness, and leaving work early. The third set looks at rehabilitation rates. The last group examines employee morale and satisfaction variables, job performance ratings, grievances, disciplinary actions. A few examples of EAP evaluation studies that have been conducted are summarized below: (1) Foote, Erfurt, Straugh, and Guzzardo (1978) conducted a detailed analysis of costs and benefits of eight programs. Significant reductions were found on the organizational outcome variables examined (e.g., absenteeism, grievances, on-the-job 19 accidents, health benefits), but they concluded that much work and more data were needed to develop an accurate and reliable cost-benefit analysis. (2) The Washington Business Group on Health (1978) concluded that most companies report benefits in improved productivity, absenteeism rates, morale, and health insurance cOsts. (3) Featherston & Bednarek (1981) reported reduced replacement, training, and unemployment insurance costs. (4) General Mills American Family Forum (1980) provided evidence Of improved productivity, morale, self-esteem, and more satisfying personal relationships and reductions in hospitalizations, medical utilization, and absenteeism. (5) DeFuentes (1986) reported improvements in absences, disciplinary actions, performance reviews, and medical leaves. Those self-referred showed more promotions, higher resolution of the problem, and higher performance ratings while those referred by others showed reduced disciplinary actions, increased absences, and reduced medical leaves. Table 1 provides a chart identifying the major EAP cost—benefit studies which were gathered by the Minnesota-based Hazelden Foundation (taken from BNA, 1987, pp. 27- 29). It seems that most evaluative studies track improvements in employment—related criteria that are easily quantifiable, such as decreased absenteeism rates, medical costs, on-the~job accidents, disability claims, grievances, and quantifiable measures of work performance. There is little information on cost savings achieved through broad-brush EAPs for employees in areas such as eating disorders, gambling compulsions, legal or financial problems, and personal relationships because these benefits are more indirect and more difficult to measure (BNA, 1987). HUHNUJUW “UHurflN-flh-Hlumoo “4‘“le it N ”HEREIN 20 an: 3 no: :3..- 3353 A- gain—80 3a: 3 one: Sec. 83.90.. .0 2.1.5.5 ”no: 2 an; EB. 3335 ”m 33:80 5.2.5:. 0.225 2.3 8 on; :3: 033.55 3 2.2.85 2 2 a: so: 3330—. Mo 33:50 ”an: 2 can. :3: 33.89.. ”a hag—:80 5.2:...- 332.. v2. 3 no; :3: 933.53. ”< bean—:50 2.2:...- ..v£::= 0.22.. o a o. as :5... 33.5"... ..¢ 239.59 23 on d 5.39.80 6 3 n. :3: 338% .0 32.600 6. 3 _ :3: 33.60.. Um b.2580 5:353 .35.... ounce: E. 3 ad :8: 3353 ..< b59590 . 2 .5. =5: 3:286 ”a Ens—:80 6 o. _ ES— 3-326 5 Enacso 5.23:. con—.5: ounce: e o. a. :8... 9.339.... .a fizz—:50 .22. 2. M... 5:22:00 9. 3 ¢.— :8: 35380 ”a Eng—=50 "a. 3 o.- Eo: 0338—. “0 2.3530 R. 3 Z :3: 923.3% ”m b.2250 3:25:- .353: ounce: a. 3 o. 5?: 34.952. ..< 33:30 no. 2 a: :8: annotfl. .: 5.39.80 6: 3 5» :3: 33.5.3 ”0 2:3an 5.2:...- Esoa 02:9: 23 3 :u :8: 9339:. ”m uni—:50 3.3. a: E 33:53 2.88.8 9.25:. .5 9:353.» .3253: B:- m=a==§ .3. 3:... {as 3 -2 3 32:38. 2 3.:— mzaacca .8. 3:... .33 3.: EE— 0338: £253.... 322. .3333 92.3.82“... ‘33 0.: 9.32. $3.3... 3.2.3 3.: 4935 ..au.a._=a£2 3.2332: 9.93 5253: 33903- oai one: 3 .52. 333. 9262—6. 35:“. 53:33 3 Run 3:- 5§=o 3333 .9 .5: 3.53.5 3:33 .328- d 32:3.- a. 3:. .aaoE< 3:33 5.320588 33.53 5 2.:— 252.3 :3. 33—5.: 3 33> 3528a 3358 32:23.6 853.35 529332: 530253.. «:55... .9 252.3 Senatori— aS; .o 3332: a .332 to! 32.03.. 3 533... cue-.Eotoa .53 3:82 :8 313.20 13.20.. 3:- 22.35 a. a: .Soaek \o 53:94 .553»; \o 2:; £35935 52.2.8: 3.3.3... 53.282 :=:oaau_ < REM \o 32:. :2: 352: 37.2 “G neon-55 312 no ESE—co «an: “a b.3500 noqiz 3 3.2.5.5 :2: 9.25.3 8938.. :— Baoaoo: 3:53 2 :3 a: .e...t2ue.3=0 «Z» . 31.: an 8.5353 o no:- 32 32:32:! .3!- no nocva< 5:35:30 a. SEE—Boo 3 3.8:: p untaouanoaua s:- 0233 033 o 3:33:39 \o 2:5. 553950,.— ..quNB. 2.... 53:3 :2 2.: .u .32:— e: — £33... an: .3335 633.: 2.2 6:33- :3 £2.30: nausea. IIIIIIIIIIIIIII pompoum uon nNEH-hung.» Adam "H oHan AhVu Ufiuouv N ”Hie-Eu!” 5:35.... 5332. 2.... .353 28.53.. 5 v3.2... :02...- ....u to: 21 8 2.253.. «6.0.9.30 b.3308... 8a.: 089...... 2.2...qu £2.85 3-2 *3 3. 39.85.53. «co—on; ISAC—.6 . on: ..—a 5....an o... 5 7......an v2.1.5: .803 8.2.0.2 3.... «5.3... 3-. .5 60...; 83.2.82 .529... 93.0.2 Sn 8.59:8 a .o .5528 .3... 3 $8.28.. 38:8. 5%.... 85¢..- uuv an 5.: 2 5.88.. 9......8. :528. 3%.... x: 3 8:38.. 2...... 5:23.... 69...: .8. .3. .3. .35.». .3... .35.: .82. o. ...a. a... $3 .33.». 3 3....— Bc..2 9.8.2095 1.3.0. 83 .o. h...- Q... 9.2.... 30:05:38. 3053......» .o 9.35 on: 4.89.3833 .3» 31.8.. a .3... .o 5.! 23...... .5559... v8.9.8 8.. gave—.3- 328... 8: 8:0:..3< 0.... .30. .< 3.9.3.. 23.2.82 .559... v8.3». 2. =0.— 39...: .....oEo.6...E. 33.....29. ES... 953. 9.3.21.5 bus-G ...5Eo»8..:.. 2.35.5.9. .552 E059... 3.3.5 so... 2.... D35...» .9 8.. v..- voog .o R». 3.3. .83 2.9.0... .0 E2... 3 o. o. b. $033.... on E... 5.. . 5503...... 2.8 Esp-o... A 8 59...... .3933. .2. a n: .08.“...3. itchonohs... .52... acuzuwuuuufiu a 8805...... 8. .- 2382... ESE...» .o a...» E.- ..oo.ooo .0 $3 853...: 3.28... a. .a. a8: a 55.382 a... q. .. .5... .1. 2.: :1 355....- votoav. 5.8.... 3.8... E22... 3.8.“ Jfinfloouu 92.3 .oz 8 39.5 :32...» .o 3: a... .688... .o .2. 8:82.... 3 .o .3: 30.5.... A: . 85.2.8... .3. a .. 8.... a o 25.. 3. 058.50 3.3.50 naggaok 23.53:. kin \o 0.5.. «2.8..utzwhfiuw atom... bx \o 598.. \o 2:: \\ .w..=ou. . m..m. Anny Utou \ V N “HANK.“ 22 .8 99...... az .. a. o 5.... 33.9.5 3. 98:2... oz 2 o. .v 5.... 33...... on. o. o... :5.— 2.3.25 3 o. 3 =5: 02....qu 3:... o o. 3:... on 5.... 9.30..qu 8...... 3 3 8:... 3. 5.... 02.825 .3... 8...... .o o. 8:... 3. 5.... 3.3.80 238...... 8a» 233...... Run 33...... .3. 933...... 83 22.3.5.9... 23.2.9. $3 .:o:.o>o....=. 23.0.... 8: 32.33.15. 23.9.... :- 39:33.95. 8..-... Ass-3...... 2.3.2. .3... a. 8: 5:3 29.2.58. 2.2.33. .8... 2.3.1.2....— 2.9.23 .33 a. s: 5.: .2358. gags... .3. 2.83.5 3:32.... :2... .3... cot-32.500 .2333 29.2. 3:... a... .3 3.333.. 22... «>3. 79...»... 3:. no.- as... vac-.23. 5.3.. 20.3 3:... 5:32.90... .95.. 3:... .o a 2.3 .83 :9. 8:... 2. a a... .3... o. .52..- .u..... .o a 83.32.... 5.3 32.23.23 £0.33... 5.... .3325... .33 2. 5.2.2.: .8: 2. 3:2... €3.33...“ 0... .0 3:2... :39... :. 32.3.5.9... 3.8.0....8 .3! x . N 356......- .<.< .8352... a... a. 30533.5. 23.2.8.3... 32.2... once-E o. 5......» .32... -38 3...... 50.22... .30...- .oao... 5.! 3:30:29. n... 235033.... 3.3.7.25 on: as... vo..o..u.-..aw 3: .22.... 23.2.... :3 333...... 2.253... .33.... 3 x .55.: a... .58... . :8 6...... 2.253... 7.2.028. 32...! a. ‘3! c 33.30....“ 2.05.3.5 5.. 59.3 s. 23. ................... 533...... 2:. ”99.3% no... 8..» 32...... a: 35.5 .7... 32.39.... 23.1. .2: 3.3.3... 3. n z 3.5.... 3.88.. .5233. 2.023.... 5.3. .555... 2... :8...- a o. 82.2.53 .- $8.3 <2. .32.... 2.3!. .o. I 2 has: 5.... 3.2.02;— .39.&@ 31H .6 0...; «2.8.3139355 3.2.5.5 \o 23.. 3.3% 02......” Av.u:oov . mHQwH Pro 23 In general, EAPs have been shown to make improvements in monetary areas since many firms have been able to decrease absenteeism by 300%, medical costs by 200%, and terminations for those using the EAP by ten times compared to those not using the EAP (Busch, 1981; Sager, 1979). Additionally, EAPs have been shown to improve non— monetary areas, such as staff morale, quality of performance, and improved relationships with coworkers (Roth, 1981). When EAP success has been defined as an improvement in job performance or in an individual’s overall functioning relationship, several companies have reported success rates ranging in improvements from 60% to 80%--E.I. DuPont de Nemours & Co. with a 66% success rate with 950 alcoholics receiving treatment; Bethlehem Steel Corporation with a 60% success rate; and Minnesota Mining and Manufacturing Company with a reported 80% success rate (Storm, 1977). Annual savings have been noted as follows at other EAPs: $2 million by the US. Postal Service, $1.5 million by the New York Telephone Company, and $5 million by DuPont (Scanlon, 1983). Otto Jones, the president of a Salt Lake City EAP, cites a return of $3.10 for every EAP dollar spent (Lovenheim, 1979). Others cite average returns of $5.78 per EAP dollar spent (Scanlon, 1983), $8.00 (United States Congress, 1982), $14.00 (at Burlington Northern Railroad), and $16.35 (at a ”major airline”) (BNA, 1987, p. 25). Wrich (1980b) found that ”companies that have developed cost effectiveness data report that the benefit to cost ratio in employee assistance programs is frequently over 1,000 percent" (p. 113). Wrich (1980b) also determined that helping all troubled employees in an organization costs about one-tenth what only one alcoholic employee will cost the Organization. While research has reported on the cost-effectiveness of most EAPs, several problems have been noted with this research. Albert, Smythe, and Brook (1985) and 24 Steidinger (1985) reviewed EAP evaluation research and practices and concluded that conceptual and methodological problems are inherent in this research and that -most studies were poorly designed and used nonostandardized performance measures. The thoroughness and validity of many studies has also been criticized. Most of the published literature concerning the success of EAPs is anecdotal and, of the empirical work, much is primarily descriptive data (J errell & Rightmyer, 1982; Kurtz, Googins, & Howard,'1984). In addition, there is a basic difficulty facing EAP evaluators which is a resistance. by EAPs and affiliated companies to conduct evaluations (Kilburg, 1980; Masi & Teems, 1983). For example, Ford and McLaughlin (1981) found that only 13 of 110 companies with an EAP calculated any dollar or productivity benefits; rather they focused primarily on changes in absenteeism rates. In a survey of EAPs by Straussner (1986), only 52% of the EAPs conducted an evaluation of their programs and the purpose was not to examine their services, but to validate the value of the program to management. Similarly, Coleman (1984) found that only 40.6% of the EAPs examined in higher educational institutions had a formal mechanism used in evaluating their programs and that only 37.5% do so annually. Finally, 34 San Francisco area EAPs were surveyed by Steidinger (1985) but only 59% reported conducting program evaluations and the majority of these were external/off—site EAPs. As previously mentioned, often employees are able to confront and resolve their problems before the problems negatively impact their work performance. However, there are times when these problems cannot be resolved and employees experience decreased job effectiveness, increased absenteeism and tardiness, and other negative effects. Methodological problems notwithstanding, EAPs can help combat these problems. lni 25 Div r i of EAP There is no typical EAP. There are variations in the EAP model utilized, structure, size, administrative policies and procedures, services provided, service delivery, funding, management support, staffing, etc. What they have in common is the basic premise that healthy employees are more productive/better employees and are valued assets and the basic goal to reduce or eliminate the basis of an employee’s impairment (Roman, 1983a). Thus, an EAP exists because the organization recognizes the value of employees and the need to maintain employees’ health (Lovenheim, 1979; Scanlon, 1983; Wrich, 1980b). EAPs are diverse in their ideological basis (Trice, 1980), although many typically are grounded in both a "humanitarian” concern for the overall well-being of employees (Trice & Roman, 1978) and a need to utilize controls to attain high productivity from employees (Trice, 1980). Diversity is also evident in the type of EAP model utilized. There are two basic EAP models. One model deals primarily with substance abuse difficulties, primarily those that continue their original focus on alcohol-related problems. The second model is referred to as "broad brush.“ The latter model provides services that deal with more than alcohol—related problems which may negatively affect job performance (Starr & Byram, 1985). Examples of ”broad-brush” services include crisis intervention, short-term counseling and/or referrals for employees experiencing difficulties related to marital problems, domestic violence, child abuse, emotional stressors, psychological disorders along with providing information on financial, legal, and vocational issues. There are also generally three basic structural models or approaches of EAPs: internal/on-site, external/off-site, or combination models. These will be discussed in more detail in chapter two. Each approach accepts a variety of referral types: self- 26 referrals, supervisor-referrals, union-referrals, significant—other referrals, or any other method by which employees come to seek assistance. It is assumed that many who are identified and referred to an EAP will again become ”good" employees. Assumptions of the Research The following assumptions are important to this study: 1. An employee’s personal and work life are interdependent; thus, some employees have problems which will affect their work performance. 2. An employer has an obligation to its employees and also to its constituencies, students, and general public, and an employer cannot adequately meet the public’s needs unless it also attends to the human needs of its employees. 3. Behavior is goal—directed. 4. Understanding an individual’s motivation to perform health-related behaviors requires identifying the expectations of future performance regarding health behaviors, the individual’s value placed on health, and the perceived benefits of health behaviors (Pender, 1982; Rotter, 1954; Wallston, Maides, & Wallston, 1976). 5. The occurrence of a behavior is determined by the nature or importance of goals or reinforcements and by a person’s expectancy that these goals will occur. 6. Employees who either participate or do not participate in EAPs have the necessary resources and equal access to participate in the EAP if they choose to do so. Organization of the Research Chapters of this dissertation are organized as follows. Chapter two provides a comprehensive review of the related EAP literature while chapter three presents a 27 discussion of both control theory in general and the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization developed in the present research. Chapter four outlines the research design and method. Chapter five presents the results and analyses of the study. Chapter six includes a summary of major conclusions, implications, and recommendations drawn from the results. In Appendix A, common terms referred to in this research are defined. Appendix B illustrates the survey instruments completed by the participants in this study. CHAPTER 2 ' REVIEW OF EAP AND HEALTHvRELATED LITERATURE Interest in work site assistance programs crosses several disciplinary fields. In 1986, Molloy indicated that "organizational and management theories have not addressed the employee assistance field per se” (p. 47). Assistance programs should fall under the human resource management (HRM) and organizational behavior (OB) fields because they focus on organizational activities that are concerned with affecting the behavior of the human resources (Ivancevich & Matteson, 1980). Since the HRM and OB fields integrate research and perspectives from several other disciplines, such as sociology, economics, psychology, and medical science, EAPs which focus on treating the physical and mental health problems of employees encompass concerns and concepts from these fields as well. Employee health, the rising costs associated with employee health, and the social and cultural variables which impact one’s health behaviors makes employee health a social, organizational, medical, economical, and psychological concern (BNA, 1983). Historical Background Historical Background to 1940--Welfare Movement Little concern for the workers and their needs seemed to exist during the early phases of the Industrial Revolution. The Industrial Revolution has been characterized by three specific developments: ”the deve10pment of machinery, the linking of human power to the machines, and the establishment of factories in which a large number of people were employed" (Cascio, 1986). Labor was considered a commodity to be 28 bought and conditions, and produo specializati noted the n less raw 1112 and tools), Smith and j only a few 5 only an op; (Cascio, 19: One 0 0fthe 19th and Was one hitder [0 d6 lead [0 p00] Chlld empk 3di'0cated b Pioneer 0f F BY the QHEd the ”i1 ititatigmu ‘ 29 bought and sold at will and the environment was characterized with dangerous working conditions, children forced to work long hours under hazardous conditions, low wages, and productivity as the primary goal. This was also a time when a great deal of job specialization developed. While Adam Smith (1776), Charles Babbage (1835) and others noted the many advantages produced by a division of labor (e.g., reduced training time, less raw materials wastage, better worker placement, greater worker expertise on tasks and tools), there were also disadvantages which we are still dealing with today. Adam Smith and Karl Marx stressed the psychological consequences of workers who perform only a few simple operations: they argued that a person becomes ignorant, is viewed as only an appendage to a machine, and exists without the need for intellectual processes (Cascio, 1986). One of the first moves toward a concern for employees came around the beginning of the 19th century. In 1799, Robert Owen became a partner in a cotton-weaving mill and was one of the first managers who believed employers and communities should work harder to develop human talents and eliminate practices that stifle individual ability and lead to poor health (Cascio, 1986). By 1810, Owen was busy attacking the practices of child employment, long working hours, and unsafe working conditions. He also advocated better housing for his apprentices and instituted one of the first performance appraisal systems (Cascio, 1986). Because of his efforts, he has been recognized as ”the pioneer of personnel management“ (Urwick, 1956). By the early 1900’s, several US. corporations became engaged in what has been called the "industrial welfare" movement (Shain, Suurvali & Boutilier, 1986), or ”welfare capitalism" (Brandes, 1976). Welfare capitalism was defined by Brandes (1976) as "any service provided for the comfort or improvement of employees which was neither a density of the industry hyanumber of factors: 2 it Campbell, 1972), e industrialization (Brant unionism (Popple, 1981) and a need to help imn tdjust(Popple, 1981). During this movem employed to help empk more women and immi them a unique set of pr beginnings of industrial sexism... [and] as bus enployees, they found tl problems; one answer w These welfare assis generally, had four basit housing of workers), ( moronic welfare (e.g., (4) personal welfare (e. 1981). Other specific Providing medical bene ill immigrants, lunch counselors (Popple, l9 30 necessity of the industry nor required by law" (p. 6). Welfare capitalism was motivated by a number of factors: a humanitarian and paternalistic concern for employees (Nelson & Campbell, 1972), an attempt to deal with social problems resulting from industrialization (Brandes, 1976), a desire to keep employees loyal and to avoid unionism (Popple, 1981), a need to reduce production costs (Nelson & Campbell, 1972), and a need to help immigrants, young workers, and females enter the workforce and adjust (Popple, 1981). During this movement, full-time welfare assistants or welfare/social secretaries were employed to help employees with personal and work—related problems, particularly as more women and immigrant groups began entering the workforce and bringing with them a unique set of problems for business. Brandes (1976) has suggested that ”the beginnings of industrial social work are rooted in what might be considered a form of sexism . . . [and] as businesses grew and employers faced growing numbers of female employees, they found themselves at a loss about treating their workers’ peculiar ’female’ problems; one answer was the hiring of ’specialists’." These welfare assistants/secretaries were often educated as teachers or nurses and, generally, had four basic roles: (1) physical welfare (e.g., health, safety, sanitation and housing of workers), (2) cultural welfare (e.g., recreation, libraries, education), (3) economic welfare (e.g., loans, pensions, hiring, firing, wage setting of employees), and (4) personal welfare (e.g., social work services for workers and their families) (POpple, 1981). Other Specific services offered during this movement included programs providing medical benefits with sick pay, clinics, doctors, nurses, schools and training for immigrants, lunch rooms, company stores, housing, recreational programs and counselors (Popple, 1981). The first recorded social secretary was Mrs. Aggie Dunn the 1,200 females in the 20;Popple, 1981, p. 16( iii large US. compan external agency for soci Paternalistic attitur designed to increase pr developed at Amoskeag community of houses, cl lor the unmarried fem; forbidding smoking and Motor Company provit problems (Bellows, 19¢ Rubber Company whicl and was only the second provided for paid vacal houriugwhich could be 1 1986). Some of the first intplemented in 1917 b bore in New York Ci boployed a psychiatrist iDawson, 1948). In r llbblems had an impac 31 hired by the H. J. Heinz Company in 1875 to interview, hire, counsel, and watch over the 1,200 females in their work setting (Miller & Coghill, as cited in Googins, 1987, p. 20; Popple, 1981, p. 160). By 1919, the Bureau of Labor Statistics reported that of the 431 large US. companies surveyed, 295 employed a welfare secretary or utilized an external agency for social work services (US. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 1919). Paternalistic attitudes are evident in an early industrial welfare program specifically designed to increase profits and decrease labor problems (and prevent unionization) developed at Amoskeag Textile Mills in Massachusetts. Amoskeag developed its own community of houses, churches, clubs, and parks and took it upon itself to act as parents for the unmarried female employees by setting curfews and establishing regulations forbidding smoking and drinking (Shain, Suurvali & Boutilier, 1986). In 1914, the Ford Motor Company provided counselors who advised employees on personal and legal problems (Bellows, 1961). Paternalistic programs were implemented by Goodyear Rubber Company which was committed to hiring and maintaining healthy employees and was only the second U.S. organization to establish a factory hospital. Goodyear also provided for paid vacations, eight—hour workdays, pensions plans, and quality family housing which could be purchased through the organization (Shain, Suurvali & Boutilier 3 1986). Some of the first programs designed specifically to help troubled employees were implemented in 1917 by Northern States Power In Minnesota and Macy’s Department Store in New York City and in 1919 by Metropolitan Life Insurance Company which employed a psychiatrist to deal with the mental health problems of employees (Bowler & Dawson, 1948). In research conducted by Macy’s staff, it was found that personal problems had an impact on the quality and quantity of workers’ job performance. As hired by the H. J. Heinz the 1,200 females in their id;l’opple, 1981, p. 160). 431 large US. companie enema] agency for social Paternalistic attitude designed to increase pro: developed at Amoskeag ' community of houses, Chl lor the unmarried femal forbidding smoking and d Motor Company provide problems (Bellows, 1961 Rubber Company which and was only the second I provided for paid vacatit housing which could be p in), Some of the first pr implemented in 1917 by Store in New York City eItlployed a psychiatrist t iDawson,1948). In r lr0b|ems had an impact: 31 hired by the H. J. Heinz Company in 1875 to interview, hire, counsel, and watch over the 1,200 females in their work setting (Miller & Coghill, as cited in Googins, 1987, p. 20; Popple, 1981, p. 160). By 1919, the Bureau of Labor Statistics reported that of the 431 large U.S. companies surveyed, 295 employed a welfare secretary or utilized an external agency for social work services (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 1919). Paternalistic attitudes are evident in an early industrial welfare program specifically designed to increase profits and decrease labor problems (and prevent unionization) developed at Amoskeag Textile Mills in Massachusetts. Amoskeag developed its own community of houses, churches, clubs, and parks and took it upon itself to act as parents for the unmarried female employees by setting curfews and establishing regulations forbidding smoking and drinking (Shain, Suurvali & Boutilier, 1986). In 1914, the Ford Motor Company provided counselors who advised employees on personal and legal problems (Bellows, 1961). Paternalistic programs were implemented by Goodyear Rubber Company which was committed to hiring and maintaining healthy employees and was only the second U.S. organization to establish a factory hospital. Goodyear also provided for paid vacations, eight-hour workdays, pensions plans, and quality family housing which could be purchased through the organization (Shain, Suurvali & Boutilier, 1986). Some of the first programs designed specifically to help troubled employees were implemented in 1917 by Northern States Power In Minnesota and Macy’s Department Store in New York City and in 1919 by Metropolitan Life Insurance Company which employed a psychiatrist to deal with the mental health problems of employees (Bowler & Dawson, 1948). In research conducted by Macy’s staff, it was found that personal problems had an impact on the quality and quantity of workers’ job performance. As cresult, the company Dawson, 1948). These p changing needs (BNA, 1‘ By the late 1920’s ti Prini f S i ni productivity. The scient emphasis on efficiency, fir and a lack of interest i Revolution had led to tr organize themselves agai adversarial relationship c nonunion sentiment by 1 programs should be distr toward the welfare prog program was abandonec management and began (Bellows, 1961). Two ad here the deaths of the p tithin these organizatio blossoms had been impl While the number of their functions moved Personnel management lid occupational menta 32 a result, the company felt a need to view employees as whole persons (Bowler & Dawson, 1948). These programs have since been expanded to meet their employees’ changing needs (BNA, 1987; LeRoux, 1982). .By the late 1920’s the growth of welfare programs began to decrease. In his book Principles of Scientific Management, Taylor (1911) outlined ways to increase productivity. The scientific management movement then began to catch on, with its emphasis on efficiency, financial motivations, an intolerance for non-productive workers, and a lack of interest in humanistic programs. At the same time, the Industrial Revolution had led to much worker discontent and fostered the need for workers to organize themselves against the abuses in the workplace. As a consequence, a strong adversarial relationship existed between labor and management during this time. The anti—union sentiment by management along with unions’ belief that the monies for these programs should be distributed to employees in the form of wages affected the attitude toward the welfare programs. In the case of the Ford Motor Co., their counseling program was abandoned after employees developed a mistrusting attitude toward management and began to resist the paternalistic interference into their private lives (Bellows, 1961). Two additional reasons for the decline in these early welfare programs were the deaths of the primary people who were involved in their initial establishment within these organizations and a decline of the textile industry where many of these programs had been implemented. While the number of welfare secretaries decreased after the first World War, many of their functions moved into two directions-one which lead to the development of the personnel management field and the other which lead to the field of industrial health and occupational mental health (Graham, 1984; Nelson & Campbell, 1972; Popple, 1981). The merging o occupational programs v QootpahomaLAimm ‘ Alcohol abuse wasr unnibuted to this abu eighteenth and nineteen employers (Meyers, 1981 breaks, similar to our cc Also, alcohol became a p saloons as a substitute f0 Addictive drinking 1 are most productive and efforts to eliminate alcol more disciplined and (1 Movement along with the alcohol from the workpl A major impetus t founding of Alcoholics Prohibition. This was als other than a "moral or dcoholism" was originall that alcoholism follows a Menage similar to conta 33 1981). The merging of elements from these two fields is the foundation of the occupational programs which are now referred to as EAPs. Occupational Alcoholism Progxams (OAPs) 0 Alcohol abuse was reported as a problem as early as 5000 BC. and employers often contributed to this abuse because consumption of alcohol on the job during the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries was not only condoned but expected by U.S. employers (Meyers, 1985). Employers offered employees wine, whiskey, and brandy breaks, similar to our coffee breaks today (Trice & Schonbrunn, 1981, pp. 172-173). Also, alcohol became a problem as immigrants and other transient workers frequented saloons as a substitute for the homes and families left behind (Brandes, 1976). Addictive drinking usually takes hold between the ages of 35 to 50 when employees are most productive and valuable to their employer. From the 1880’s to the 1920’s, efforts to eliminate alcohol from the workplace were undertaken in order to build a more disciplined and dependable workforce. During this time, the Temperance Movement along with the emergence of workmen’s compensation laws helped to remove alcohol from the workplace (Trice & Schonbrunn, 1981, pp. 173-174). A major impetus to the revival of corporate counseling programs came after the founding of Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) in 1935, two years after the repeal of prohibition. This was also a time when alcoholism first began to be viewed as a sickness rather than a "moral or spiritual deficiency" (BNA, 1987). This “disease concept of alcoholism” was originally introduced by Dr. E. M. J ellinek in 1939 whose theory stated that alcoholism follows a pattern of progressive psychological and physiological bodily damage similar to contagious diseases (Jellinek, 1960). Defining alcoholism as a disease — idped pave the way for Itporteti as the most wit cuts across all profession Roman, 1978). With the help of A} jobs and improve their v of occupational alcoholi employees and providing (Masi, 1984). These 1 employees. The threat limiting to a problem a During this time, t wheeling services. In 1‘. providing services relate recreational work, housir Some of the first eloployees with alcohol— Co, BasnnaneKodak Consolidated Edison of Co.(BNA, 1987; Roman, brooch humanitarian an “i an increase. Simil ccGnostic advantages, w “i a desire to save jobs 34 helped pave the way for medical intervention in its treatment. Today alcohol has been reported as the most widely used mood-altering drug in the U.S. and is a problem that cuts across all professions and occupations equally (Pattison & Kaufman, 1983; Trice & Roman, 1978). With the help of AA, alcoholic employees were able to maintain sobriety and their jobs and improve their work productivity. The success of AA led to the establishment of occupational alcoholism programs (OAPs) which focused on recognizing alcoholic employees and providing help before their reduced performance resulted in termination (Masi, 1984). These programs were initially supervised by recovered alcoholic employees. The threat of job loss had a major impact on an alcoholic employee admitting to a problem and seeking help. During this time, the Federal Government also began implementing employee counseling services. In 1938, the Social Security Board established a counseling program providing services related to guidance, psychiatric and social work, personnel and recreational work, housing and transportation problems (Bowler & Dawson, 1948). Some of the first OAPs that were established in the mid-1940’s to rehabilitate employees with alcohol—related difficulties were offered by El. DuPont de Nemours & Co., Eastman-Kodak Co., Kemper Insurance, Allis-Chalmers Manufacturing Co., Consolidated Edison of New York, Standard Oil of New Jersey, and Caterpillar Tractor Co.(BNA, 1987 ; Roman, 1980a). Implementation of the DuPont program was prompted by both humanitarian and economic motives since the termination rate of employees was on an increase. Similarly, management’s interest at Allis-Chalmers focused on the economic advantages, while the union’s interest was based on a humanitarian concern and a desire to save jobs (Baxter, 1978). Impressive results were often reported with — these early programs. 1 employees treated for al World War II was him conditions at the t skilled workers was Iimi which resulted in the hi work with them (BNA, 1 problems, organizations employees. Long work 1 horror demands, and int problems which interfere Consequently, mental he help integrate workers is Occupational Alcr employees and encourag during the post World W who wanted to share th Programs claimed an av hogan to slow down (M their mental health prog Programs remained sma Several reasons ace responsibility to identi Participation in the org 35 these early programs. For example, Eastman Kodak reported that 75% of the 3000 employees treated for alcoholism were rehabilitated (Norris, 1948). World War II was another strong force in the establishment of OAPs due to the labor conditions at the time. Industries were forced to mass produce, but selection of skilled workers was limited so companies were forced to lower employment standards, which resulted in the hiring of more workers who brought their diverse problems to work with them (BNA, 1987). Additionally, as soldiers returned with alcohol and other problems, organizations realized they needed to take an active part in helping employees. Long work hours and shift work, pressure to improve productivity to meet the war demands, and inexperienced supervisors unable to deal with employees’ personal problems which interfered with their job performance further exacerbated the problems. Consequently, mental health and social service programs in industry were established to help integrate workers into the workplace (BNA, 1987; Lewis, 1981). Occupational Alcoholism Programs were established to identify alcoholic employees and encourage them to seek treatment. The early programs in existence during the post World War II era often depended on the fervor of recovered alcoholics who wanted to share their sobriety with other employees (Baxter, 1978). While these programs claimed an average recovery rate of 60-70 percent, growth of such programs began to slow down (Masi, 1982). After the war, the majority of companies shut down their mental health programs, and throughout the 1950’s the number of businesses with Programs remained small" (Sonnenstuhl & Trice, 1986 cited in BNA, 1987). Several reasons account for this slow growth. First, it was typically the supervisors’ responsibility to identify employees with drinking problems and encourage their participation in the organization’s program. This method worked well in identifying — rank-and-file personnel of the working populati because alcoholism was attempted to hide their ( ass witch-hunt of non-i prevailed (Masi, 1984). confronted, they typical Fourth, often there is a 5 about making accusation sometimes feared that b' they have an alcoholism wasalack of available re (Masi, 1984). Thus, l occupational counselin rehabilitation programs During the 1960’s, factors influenced this gr alcohol symptoms to im alcoholism as a disease. extruding and a concer developed in order to se influencing this search f hellth agencies over—tax awareness of the influe 36 rank-and—file personnel but not managerial-level employees. Therefore, a large segment of the working population was not only ignored, but management avoided treatment because alcoholism was seen as afflicting only the lower echelons. Second, employees attempted to hide their drinking problem since they viewed a supervisor’s confrontation as a witch-hunt of non-managerial personnel, and thus an us—versus-them atmosphere prevailed (Masi, 1984). Third, alcoholics not only deny they have problems but when confronted, they typically blame their problem on anyone or anything but themselves. Fourth, often there is a social stigma attached to alcoholism so supervisors were hesitant about making accusations until the problem was in advanced stages. Fifth, organizations sometimes feared that by offering a visible program they would be acknowledging that they have an alcoholism problem in the organization (Holden, 1973). Sixth, often there was a lack of available resources and of trained professionals to administer the programs (Masi, 1984). Thus, by 1959, only 50 American companies had implemented occupational counseling programs--35 which focused exclusively on alcohol rehabilitation programs (Carr & Hellan, 1980; Masi, 1984). During the 1960’s, however, the number of OAPs began to grow. Three major factors influenced this growth-growth in community health services, a shift in focus from alcohol symptoms to impaired job performance and work behaviors, and acceptance of alcoholism as a disease. First, during the 1960’s community mental services were greatly expanding and a concern about developing other effective, briefer methods of therapy developed in order to serve more people. Gelso and Johnson (1983) cite several factors influencing this search for other methods: increased demands for service had left mental health agencies over-taxed; long waiting lists resulted in service delays; heightened public awareness of the influence psychological factors have on human functioning caused — many types of people t populations other than pay for mental health tr treatments. The second impetu refocus on a "job perfor performance of employ occupational programs. alcoholism symptoms. ”l the uncomfortable role performance was manife employer could legitima Supervisors were now tr: alcoholic behaviors. Joh toasupervisor’s subjecti not directly accusing thi watchful in getting th problem-drinking empl The confrontational app to motivate workers to “rice, 1976). Finally, the “diseas Medical Association in (Follman,1978). Viewi 37 many types of people to seek treatment; mental health services became available to populations other than the middle class; and demands on health insurance policies to pay for mental health treatment lead to insurance companies examining the efficacy of treatments. The second impetus for OAP growth was that, during this time, organizations began to focus on a "job performance model" where dysfunctional work behaviors and the job performance of employees were used to help identify those who might be helped by occupational programs. These behaviors were believed to be less stigmatic than alcoholism symptoms. The focus on work behaviors also removed the supervisor from the uncomfortable role of diagnostician (Wrich, 1980b). Since deterioration in job performance was manifested in the early stages of some problems (e.g., alcoholism), the employer could legitimately intervene to bring about change (Roman & Trice, 1976). Supervisors were now trained to observe declining job performance rather than to spot alcoholic behaviors. Job behaviors are more difficult for employees to deny compared to a supervisor’s subjective evaluation. By focusing on declining job performance and not directly accusing the employee of being an alcoholic, supervisors may be more successful in getting the employee to admit to having a drinking problem. Since problem—drinking employees must support their habit, they don’t want to lose their job. The confrontational approach toward employees’ job performance was viewed as a way to motivate workers to help themselves and modify their destructive behavior (Roman & Trice, 1976). Finally, the "disease model" of alcoholism was officially accepted by the American Medical Association in 1956 (Masi, 1984), and in 1959 the AFL-CIO followed suit (Follman, 1978). Viewing alcoholism as a treatable disease moved organizations away — from taking a punitive The new approach was previously mentioned th OAPs were the basis 0 emotional, psychologicr impacting work perform 1 0’ Fr r In a 1979 survey, broader-focused EAPs. t to family members of e occurred to further inflt For example, the er into the late 1960’s ant initially, industry‘s respr that of alcoholism; i.e. Mutated of abusing dr in addition to the enhanced due to the leg prohibiting discriminati Rehabilitation Act of 19 the definition of "ha humane approach to dru (it, Department of Def 38 from taking a punitive approach to one that was more positive toward the employee. The new approach was coined "tough love,” which used the confrontational strategy previously mentioned that focused on breaking down an alcoholic’s denial system. Thus, OAPs were the basis of the development of “broad-brush" EAPs which address the emotional, psychological, and social needs of employees and problems negatively impacting work performance. The 1970’s to the Present--The Legal Influences on EAPs In a 1979 survey, Roman (19803) found that most OAPs were housed in the broader-focused EAPs. Occupational programs were also expanded to include assistance to family members of employees. During the late 1960’s and 1970’s, several forces occurred to further influence the growth and development of EAPs. For example, the evolution from alcoholism to other personal problems continued into the late 1960’s and 1970’s as drug abuse became more of a societal problem. Initially, industry’s response to the increased occurrence of drug abuse was similar to that of alcoholism; i.e., initial denial or punitive actions toward those employees suspected of abusing drugs (Johnston, 1971; Rush & Brown, 1971; Stevens, 1970). In addition to the types of problems addressed by EAPs, the growth of EAPs was enhanced due to the legal climate during the 1970’s. Federal legislation in the 1970’s prohibiting discrimination against handicapped individuals, such as the Vocational Rehabilitation Act of 1973, which was extended to include alcoholics and drug abusers in the definition of "handicapped”, was a major force in the development of a more humane approach to drug abuse (Vicary & Resnik, 1982, p. 16). The federal government (e.g., Department of Defense) also established programs during this time which focused on education, rehabilitz The Hughes Act, 0: Treatment and Rehabil resurgence of occupatio. (National Institute on 1 Education, and Welfare private and public sect alcoholism programs in abuse was included (M funding to pay for servic programs (Masi, 1984). Several factors infl 1980’s. First, there wa: where employers were I problems as a result of co financially responsible in iul9ll affirmative actior drug addicts and alcol ldtuuations expanded psychotherapy. Fourth iilliterate benefits and p! 1987iTersine & Hazeldil ill Health Act, equal torsion legislation alst 39 on education, rehabilitation, and treatment (Korcok & Seidler, 1978). The Hughes Act, or the Comprehensive Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Prevention, Treatment and Rehabilitation Act, is also considered to be a major impetus to the resurgence of occupational programs during the 1970’s. This law established the NIAAA (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism) in the Department of Health, Education, and Welfare to coordinate efforts in combatting alcohol problems in both the private and public sectors. The Hughes Act also mandated the implementation of alcoholism programs in all federal agencies and military installations. In 1972, drug abuse was included (Masi, 1984). Through the NIAAA, states could obtain federal funding to pay for services of trained consultants who could help establish rehabilitation programs (Masi, 1984). Several factors influenced the tremendous growth of EAPs during the 1970’s and 1980’s. First, there was a broadening of worker’s compensation/handicap coverage where employers were held more liable for employees’ alcohol, drug, and emotional problems as a result of court rulings and arbitration decisions. Employers were also held financially responsible for on-the-job accidents regardless of fault (BNA, 1987). Second, in 1977 affirmative action programs were implemented requiring the hiring of qualified drug addicts and alcoholics by federal contractors and subcontractors. Third, organizations expanded the benefits offered to employees to include counseling and psychotherapy. Fourth, unions began demanding alcoholism and mental health insurance benefits and programs as part of their collective bargaining agreements (BNA, 1987; Tersine & Hazeldine, 1982, pp. 69-70). Fifth, passage of the Occupational Safety and Health Act, equal employment opportunity legislation, and environmental protection legislation also spurred a change in corporate attitudes and culture toward a greater concern about employees. Additional forces I the 1970’s are discussed The 1910’s to the PIQQI During this period job performance. Cons personal problems such 1 problems, retirement, as Thain & Groeneveld, 19 We have also move workers-workers who t conditions compared to The shift in the stn produce more thar enough. Work also The focus of EAPs was a the realization that techn American industry spent he human element-whr fiduciary-industry bl hippie who operate tht hundred because thei toilets experience inert 40 greater concern about employers’ social responsibility for their communities and employees. Additional forces cited as influencing the growth and development of EAPs since the 1970’s are discussed in the following section. The 1920’s to the Present-~Additional Influences on EAPs During this period other problems were recognized as having a negative impact on job performance. Consequently, EAPs focused on alcoholism, drug abuse, and other personal problems such as depression, phobias, divorce, domestic violence, child-rearing problems, retirement, anxiety, relocation issues, etc. (BNA, 1987; Carr & Hellan, 1980; Shain & Groeneveld, 1980; Trice & Roman, 1972). We have also moved towards a technological society that requires highly skilled workers--workers who tend to hold different values regarding their job and working conditions compared to the factory worker of the past. In 1974, Drucker stated: The shift in the structure and character of work has created a demand that work produce more than purely economic benefits. To make a living is no longer enough. Work also has to make a life (p. 179). The focus of EAPs was also broadened as a result of increased foreign competition and the realization that technology is not enough to maintain high productivity levels. While American industry spent a great deal of time automating factories and trying to eliminate the human element-~which was believed to be a major cause of error and decreased productivity-industry began realizing that machines are not enough; rather, it is the people who operate the machines who are valuable resources that must also be maintained because they are so costly to train and replace (Lewis, 1981). Also, as workers experience increased alienation from the workplace and as limited dollars are available for salary incr concerned with the ove In 1979, there wer sector (DHHS, 1982). additional 1,000 outside by 1987 over 10,000 E) which included 80% of of most programs today the program and to rel rather than performanc increasing the professior to improve their ability (llirkland, 1983; Forrest EAPs have typical! and other substance-abs increase in the number a approach taken by organ refunds. Thus, the EA! hnsidcntify troubled c three, and it is aimed at Groeneveld, 1980). 41 available for salary increases, both employers and employees have had to become more concerned with the overall quality of life within the workplace. In 1979, there were over 5,000 EAPs in the private sector and 677 in the public sector (DHHS, 1982). By 1983 the total figure had increased to 8,000 EAPs with an additional 1,000 outside consultants providing a variety of services (Roman, 1983b), and by 1987 over 10,000 EAPs were operating in the U.S. (BNA, 1987; Champion, 1988), which included 80% of the Fortune 500 companies (BNA, 1987). The broader concern of most programs today is considered to result in less stigma attached to those utilizing the program and to relieve the supervisor of having a primary role as diagnostician rather than performance evaluator (Wrich, 1980b). Organizations have also been increasing the professionalism of the counseling staff within the EAP and their training to improve their ability to handle and/or refer employees with all types of problems (Birkland, 1983; Forrest, 1983). EAPs have typically relied on supervisory referrals to get employees with alcohol and other substance-abuse problems to seek treatment. However, as a result of the increase in the number of problems handled by EAPs today and the more humanistic approach taken by organizations, a greater emphasis has been placed on employee self- referrals. Thus, the EAP of today has a dual focusnit is used in the workplace to help firms identify troubled employees and control problems such as alcoholism and drug abuse, and it is aimed at helping employees with a broad range of problems (Shain & Groeneveld, 1980). The Wellness Concept The broad-brush: many programs include programs-in addition tr typical substance abuse under the more tradit programs may be offerr nutrition, fitness, stress variety of formats (e.g., amount of evidence tha Belloc & Breslow, 1972; Therefore, in addition I problems, organizations and maintain a healthy 1 lounger, 1987; Perham The relationship be wellness programs have a The success of corporate the down, maintain he touhled employees typit 1932; Levine, 1983). TI iflied "mega-brush" prol her 50,000 organization! l”Blinds, 1983). 42 W The broad-brush approach to EAPs has most recently expanded to the point where many programs include a third component—employee wellness or prevention—oriented programs-~in addition to the rehabilitative and disciplinary components. Along with the typical substance abuse, financial, child-rearing, stress, and other problems handled under the more traditional broad-brush EAPs discussed above, a variety of other programs may be offered under the wellness umbrella. Such programs may include nutrition, fitness, stress management, and weight control which may be offered in a variety of formats (e.g., classes, lectures, workshops, and brochures). There is a great amount of evidence that lifestyle factors influence one’s health status (Belloc, 1973; Belloc & Breslow, 1972; Fielding, 1984b; Palmore, 1971; Public Health Service, 1979). Therefore, in addition to their continued interest in helping employees with existing problems, organizations are now emphasizing the need to deter problems and promote and maintain a healthy lifestyle among employees (Brink, 1983; Goodstadt, Simpson & Loranger, 1987; Perham, 1984). The relationship between one’s health behavior and health status is evident, and wellness programs have attempted to influence a variety of employee health behaviors. The success of corporate wellness programs has come in their ability to keep company costs down, maintain healthy employees, and to reach many more workers than the troubled employees typically assisted under the broad-brush concept (J euchter & Utne, 1982; Levine, 1983). These expanded programs are developing into what have been called "mega—brush" programs (Delaney, 1983). Some have estimated that there are now over 50,000 organizations involved in some type of work site health promotion (Howe, 1983; Jacobs, 1983). — Slum f o l The above section EAPs in the workplace have evolved over the helpful to summarize ti and their health. llgen and colleagu Swisher, 1989) describe health at work. These h of employee health. Fi each. A brief discussion for the current focus on in the early 1900’ primarily concerned witl was given to employee ht llgen (1990), the first m result of public concern 1 he'hafety" model since conditions of employees only the formation of g( conditions (e.g., the Nat: hmpensation plans whic his], health was viewe« hunter on the worker. 43 Summary of Models/Attitudes Regarding Employee Health The above sections have provided an historical overview of the development of EAPs in the workplace and outlined how employers’ attitudes toward employee health have evolved over the years. Before continuing the discussion of EAPs, it might be helpful to summarize the evolutionary process that has occurred regarding employees and their health. Ilgen and colleagues (Hollenbeck, Ilgen & Crampton, 1990; llgen, 1990; llgen & Swisher, 1989) described five general models or approaches that have been applied to health at work. These have progressed from a focus on safety to a broader systems view of employee health. Figure 1 outlines the basic models and the primary concerns of each. A brief discussion of the models will follow to provide an understanding and basis for the current focus on a systems perspective to health. in the early 1900’s when scientific management was popular, employers were primarily concerned with the productivity and efficiency of employees and little concern was given to employee health and problems. According to Ilgen and Swisher (1989) and Ilgen (1990), the first model of health at work emerged in the 1930’s and 1940’s as a result of public concern over dangerous working conditions. This first model was called the ”safety" model since its primary focus was on the job environment and working conditions of employees (Ilgen, 1990; Ilgen & Swisher, 1989). This model was spurred on by the formation of government agencies formed specifically to address safe working conditions (e.g., the National Safety Council) along with the development of workers’ compensation plans which aided employees injured on the job (llgen, 1990). Under this model, health was viewed as unidirectional--factors from the job environment have an impact on the worker. — Summary of ModelszAtt The above sections EAPs in the workplace a have evolved over the y helpful to summarize the and their health. llgen and colleague Swisher, 1989) described health at work. These ha of employee health. Fig each. Abrief discussion c for the current focus on 2 in the early 1900’s primarily concerned with wasgiven to employee bez lien (1990), the first moi result of public concern 0 he"snfety" model since conditions of employees My the formation of go Conditions (e.g., the Nati cOlttpensation plans whic Model, health was viewed littlest on the worker. 43 Summary of Models/Attitudes Regarding Employee Health The above sections have provided an historical overview of the development of EAPs in the workplace and outlined how employers’ attitudes toward employee health have evolved over the years. Before continuing the discussion of EAPs, it might be helpful to summarize the evolutionary process that has occurred regarding employees and their health. llgen and colleagues (Hollenbeck, llgen & Crampton, 1990; llgen, 1990; llgen & Swisher, 1989) described five general models or approaches that have been applied to health at work. These have progressed from a focus on safety to a broader systems view of employee health. Figure 1 outlines the basic models and the primary concerns of each. A brief discussion of the models will follow to provide an understanding and basis for the current focus on a systems perspective to health. in the early 1900’s when scientific management was popular, employers were primarily concerned with the productivity and efficiency of employees and little concern was given to employee health and problems. According to Ilgen and Swisher (1989) and llgen (1990), the first model of health at work emerged in the 1930’s and 1940’s as a result of public concern over dangerous working conditions. This first model was called the "safety" model since its primary focus was on the job environment and working conditions of employees (Ilgen, 1990; Ilgen & Swisher, 1989). This model was spurred on by the formation of government agencies formed specifically to address safe working conditions (e.g., the National Safety Council) along with the development of workers’ compensation plans which aided employees injured on the job (Ilgen, 1990). Under this model, health was viewed as unidirectional--factors from the job environment have an impact on the worker. — rode 1 term "Jerking Candida EARLY ERGONOHICS Working Condition MESS Life Style<-,\ All MEMO“. Iflma/Injuryéd Work Environment: Social. Systems (S Indtvdual. System: no nnnnnnnnnnnnnnn 09¢ Source: Hollenbeck figure 1: Approach A 44 Hodel. target of: Concern SAFETY 'Qoricing Condition“ Workers Job Environment (Safe Worktng Conditions) EARLY ERCONOHICS . tJoric:l.ng Conditions (o) Workers Job Envaronment (Safe Working Conditions and Safe working Behaviors) 'JELL‘JESS Life Styla All. Employees Employee Behaviors (Diet. Smoking, Substance Abuse) HEDICAL Illness/Injury“) All. Employees Physical. Syntons HEALTH Work Environments (WE) -- -- WE All Components in Social Systems (33) {S‘5 SS Interaction Indivfiual. Systems (IS) .-- -- IS oooooooooooooooooooo coo-o-scoooooooooooooooooooo.90---.ooooooooooooooooooo- Source: Hollenbeck, Ilgen & Crampton, 1990 Figure 1: Approaches To Health At Work The impetus for It model-came about with with employees, improvt on the environment bu incentives provided to en olindividuals. This new conditions played a majc regulations as a result of ' ergonomics model coincit 1950’s for the alcohol-rel the development of CAP A5 Previously discu employees is important society. Consequently 45 The impetus for the development of the second model--the ”early ergonomics" model--came about with the realization that since work environments interact so closely with employees, improvement in safe working conditions must include not only a focus on the environment but also on the safe working behaviors of the workforce, the incentives provided to encourage safe behaviors, and the safe selection and/or placement of individuals. This new bi-directional relationship between workers and their working conditions played a major role in training workers in safe behaviors and in developing regulations as a result of the Occupational Health and Safety Act (OSHA) of 1970. The ergonomics model coincides with the greater occupational concern during the 1940’s and 1950’s for the alcohol-related problems of employees-—which, as outlined above, lead to the development of OAPs (Occupational Alcoholism Programs). As previously discussed, eventually employers realized that the overall health of employees is important to the performance of the employee, the organization, and society. Consequently, EAPs, wellness programs, and other occupational health promotion programs developed in the 1970’s and 1980’s as a dual concern evolved to include not only the prevention of injuries but the encouragement of healthy physical and psychological life-styles (Brink, 1983; Delaney, 1983; Goodstadt, Simpson & Loranger, 1987; llgen, 1990; Perham, 1984). This third approach toward employee health is referred to as the "wellness" approach because it focuses on the inter- relationships between employee behaviors (e.g., diet, exercise, smoking, substance abuse) and their life-style, rather than simply focusing on job conditions as the causes of health problems (Ilgen, 1990; llgen & Swisher, 1989). The fourth model, according to llgen and Swisher (1989), actually predated the above three and is still in use today. This is known as the "medical" model and focuses — on the physical symptor injury or are ill. The w from one or a few defin health problems today at psychological, social, el dealing with employee 1 The last model or r by emphasizing the inter. individual systems (Holl systems consist of indivi beliefs and communicatic tothehiopsychosocial n consideration the biologit impact on the health of \ Humans are influeni scholar, organ, organ s liflyder,1989). These re of complexity, defined as liSwisher, 1989). To un heeded one has on oth tonne the lower levels 00Illtllurrity, culture, etc.) he focus of the past and “lifted, biolpsycho/soci 46 on the physical symptoms of workers who are referred to physicians when they have an injury or are ill. The weakness of this approach is that illnesses are assumed to result from one or a few definable causes, which can be isolated and treated. However, many health problems today are caused by a complex set of interdependent systems (biological, psychological, social, etc.) and thus a broader systems perspective is required when dealing with employee health. The last model or approach to health at work takes on the "systems" perspective by emphasizing the inter-relationships among the work environments, social systems, and individual systems (Hollenbeck et al., 1990; llgen, 1990; llgen & Swisher, 1989). Social systems consist of individuals interacting with others according to a shared system of beliefs and communication means (Wiseman, 1966). The ”systems“ perspective is similar to the biopsychosocial model of medicine espoused by Engel (1977) which takes into consideration the biological, psychological, and social environments and their combined impact on the health of workers. Humans are influenced by and made up of many organizational levels--molecular, cellular, organ, organ system, psychological, behavioral, environmental, social, etc. (Snyder, 1989). These levels are arranged hierarchically in Figure 2 in ascending order of complexity, defined as the number of interactions possible at a specific level (llgen & Swisher, 1989). To understand the major influences impacting on an employee and the effect one has on other levels, we must take on a broader systems perspective and examine the lower levels (systems, organs, tissues, etc.) and the upper levels (family, community, culture, etc.) which man frequently interacts with. Figure 2 also identifies the focus of the past and current perspectives (i.e., bio-medical approach, psycho/social approach, bio/psycho/social approach) in examining health and work behaviors. Psycho/Social Approach F? l Rio Medical Approach Source: Ilge Figure 2: Hi 47 RIERARCHY OF m AND SCIENCE BIOSPHERE HOHOSAP {ENS SOCI {TY/NATION CULTURE Psycho/Social Approach l SUBCULTURE CWUN IT? FAMILY‘ . Bi «Psycho/Social Behavior P5150» (Levels. of Conduct and Expert ente) SYSTEHS Bio Hedi cat Approach: ORGANS TISSUES CELLS r OfiWELLS HOLECULES AIOHS . suaArontc PARTICLES ounaxs Source: Ilgen & Swisher, 1989 Figure 2: Hierarchy of Man and Science — This evolution in evolution of EAPs. Tc toward employee heal environmental, psycholt the employee’s health it environments. This systems perSpt Utilization for the press remainder of this chap current status and struct Themgmrg and Dryer As mentioned in 1 depending on the organiz There are no guidelines individual programs. E policies established, de delivery, depending to a Regardless of the minagernent support an Management and labor ( ii key to obtaining em p lttypical assessment hemmed by Wrich ( —7 48 This evolution in the approaches toward health at work coincides with the evolution of EAPs. Today, industry has taken on this broader "systems" perspective toward employee health by recognizing that many factors (e.g., biological, social, environmental, psychological, etc.) influence an employee’s overall health and, in turn, the employee’s health influences all areas of his/her life-both the work and non-work environments. This systems perspective has been applied in the development of the Model of EAP Utilization for the present research, which will be presented in the next chapter. The remainder of this chapter will continue the discussion of EAPs by focusing on the current status and structure of EAPS and specific areas of research. The Structure and Diversity of EAPs As mentioned in chapter one, EAPs today are very diverse in composition, depending on the organizational setting, needs, and the labor-management relationship. There are no guidelines or mandates on the structure, functioning, or breadth of individual programs. Employee Assistance Programs vary in the model implemented, policies established, degree of management support, services provided, and service delivery, depending to a great extent on the size of the organization and employee needs. Regardless of the model implemented, the most successful EAPs have t0p management support and are jointly designed, implemented, and maintained by both management and labor (Beyer & Trice, 1978; Minter, 1983). In addition, confidentiality is a key to obtaining employee trust and utilization. A flow-chart of activities involved in a typical assessment and referral process, regardless of the model utilized, is diagrammed by Wrich (1980b) in Figure 3. While standardized procedures do exist, the . _ w . - 5.40: flu i535!“ "ho it‘d Ifl’orni C'hvflwdb: Dun-300‘ Iflbul 01": .IID" IIUhI‘ II>O:II nINUL Jen's-u! 20.13 hdoanQ “Nguuxpuonvdw HQVI MO Input-”Gun 83°F“! mofluw>fiuu< mtwhaw . . ice) n25 a” 22933:... u... q _ 3?... 3321. 33.533. 3:. 1 3‘3, ”.933 L - 2...; ‘3UZOU nmuaanfiuc urab-U.BS5...— — process an employee m3 problem (Wrich, 1980b and possess the necess: employee and recommt The two most popr the "broad-brush" EA] Regarding specific servi services that can be c Administration includes and evaluating the prog appropriate treatment n provide management co As stated earlier, administration of EAPs employee who has a prol confidentiality) and Management’s attitude structure, as well as the both geographically and nEAP’s success. Mas' hderlhe personnel fun Nearly the concern of SO process an employee may take within an EAP is individualized according to the worker’s problem (Wrich, 1980b). The EAP counselor must understand the resources available and possess the necessary assessment, diagnostic, and clinical skills for evaluating an employee and recommending treatment. The two most popular models of service for employee counseling are the CAP and the "broad-brush” EAP model, both discussed above (Forrest, 1983; Wrich, 1980b). Regarding specific services offered, Winkelpleck (1984) states that most EAPs provide services that can be classified into two functions: administration and counseling. Administration includes activities such as implementing, marketing, staffing, maintaining and evaluating the program. Counseling includes problem assessment and referral to appropriate treatment resources. In addition to these functions, some EAPs may also provide management consultation, education, and organizational development. As stated earlier, confidentiality is a critical issue influencing the successful administration of EAPs (Masi, 1984; Myers, 1984; Wrich, 1974, 1980a). Since it is the employee who has a problem, it is his or her perceptions of the EAP policies (including confidentiality) and management’s attitude toward the EAP that are critical. Management’s attitude can be influenced by how the EAP fits into the organizational structure, as well as the physical location of the EAP; therefore, the location of the EAP both geographically and within the organization’s structure is often a key issue impacting an EAP’s success. Masi (1984, p. 34) argues that "it is vital that the EAP be located under the personnel functions . . . (since) EAPs are connected to job performance which is clearly the concern of personnel." The location of EAP services within an organization’s hierarchy has also changed over the years. Early programs focusing primarily on alcohol-related problems were typically started and d most programs fall und professionals such as 5 location of an EAP \l Sponsorship of the EA alternatives (i.e., spor organization assumes a performs some function for-service basis which 0rionization pays an on lorsupervisor training a sen-ices rendered which 0ltanization pays a flat insurance carrier for 3pc ft is also critical to EAP should be access confidentiality (Masi 1 lllustratedb e exister E Al’s. We really do not b lHung (1988) on exi 51 typically started and directed by a company’s medical department. Today, however, most programs fall under the personnel/industrial relations division and are staffed by professionals such as social workers and psychologists (Erfurt & Foote, 1977). The location of an EAP within an organization’s hierarchy is often influenced by the sponsorship of the EAP. According to Masi (1984), there are basically four funding alternatives (i.e., sponsorship approaches) available to an organization: (1) the organization assumes all expenses and maintains its own staff; (2) the organization performs some functions but contracts out for referral and counseling services on a fee- for-service basis which is covered by the organization’s insurance carrier; (3) the organization pays an outside contractor a flat administrative fee per employee (typically for supervisor training and administrative management by the contractor) plus a fee for services rendered which are covered by the organization’s insurance carrier; or (4) the organization pays a flat fee to an outside contractor, who is not reimbursed by an insurance carrier for specific services rendered. [t is also critical to the success of an EAP to consider its physical location. The EAP should be accessible by all employees and should be located to maximize confidentiality (Masi, 1984). Geographical diversity in the structure of EAPs is illustrated by the existence of both on-site, or internal, EAPs and off-site, or external, EAPs. We really do not know which approach is more critical. ln research conducted by Hung (1988) on existing EAPs, approximately an equal distribution was found between internal/on-site and external/off—site programs. The physical location of an EAP is often influenced by the sponsorship or funding of the EAP. In addition to the internal and external programs, other methods of providing EAP services include a hybrid of both the internal and external models, a union model, peer model, the 800-number, and the consortium model- The internal mod full- or part-time coun coordinate employees’ 1 typically provide servic subsequent referrals to short-term counseling recommended (BNA, programs: EAP counse working environment, ( isavital factor in the sur easier for employees wh concerns about confider the Regional Manager 0 concerns may not be eli External pf0glams lorservices which are Dr trample, COPE (Center DC. provides EAP sen 3 (MW). External a . metrme remove the E to . ' inmunrcatton that is 11E aI fin order to justify a mini Um number of 2,( 52 the consortium model. Each of these will be discussed in more detail below. The internal model is used mostly in larger organizations which employ either a full— or part-time counseling staff that functions within an organization’s structure to coordinate employees’ needs with treatment resources (Myers, 1984). Internal programs typically provide services in-house, which usually include employee assessment with subsequent referrals to external community services. The programs may also undertake short-term counseling with referral to outside providers if longer-term treatment is recommended (BNA, 1987). Researchers cite the following advantages of on-site programs: EAP counselors are often more attuned to a company’s needs and to the working environment, on-site programs tend to have more managerial support—-which is a vital factor in the success of an EAP (Fabricatore & Rogal, 1984), and access is often easier for employees when the EAP is in close proximity to their workplace. However, concerns about confidentiality and trust may be greater with on-site programs, though the Regional Manager of Counseling Services for AT&T, Dan Caliendo, indicates these concerns may not be eliminated even with off—site programs (BNA, 1987). External programs are those in which typically smaller organizations contract out for services which are provided primarily off-site by a large, multi-service provider. For example, COPE (Center for Occupational Programs for Employees, Inc.) in Washington, DC. provides EAP services for 45 companies with approximately 35,000 employees (BNA, 1987). External programs insure a great amount of confidentiality, but at the same time remove the EAP from the daily operations of the company and the ongoing communication that is needed with employees (Lewis, 1981). McClellan (1985) suggests that in order to justify an on-site full-time EAP worker, the work site must employ a minimum number of 2,000 workers. However, 80% of all non-government workers today are in work sil contracting-011i We“ program costs (BNA, 1' such as the legal advanl up time for companies marketing methods of .1 Combination inter organizations spread or providea certain level ( in close enough proxim small in size to justify a f EAP providers (BNA, 1 While the above a] markets and work popu EAPs supported jointly President of the AFL~C the Well being of their Ir. referral program, such . 53 today are in work sites with less than 100 employees; therefore, the external, or contracting—out system, has become the fastest growing EAP model because of the lower program costs (BN A, 1987). Straussner (1985) cites other benefits of external programs, such as the legal advantages of a program that exists outside the organization, faster start- up time for companies using contractors who have established programs, and aggressive marketing methods of EAP contractors. Combination internal/external approaches to providing services are often used by organizations spread out in several geographic locations. These programs are able to provide a certain level of treatment internally by a professional staff to those employees in close enough proximity of the services while in other locations, which usually are too small in size to justify a full—time staff, contractual relationships are established with local EAP providers (BNA, 1987). While the above approaches are common, other approaches exist to serve specific markets and work populations. In 1979, the AFL-CIO adopted a resolution in favor of EAPs supported jointly by labor and management. According to Lane Kirkland, president of the AF L-CIO, "American trade unions have a fundamental concern with the well being of their members and their families” (Myers, 1984, p. 54). A union/peer referral program, such as the one in use by the Association of Flight Attendants, is supported by the union and comprised of union and employee/peer members who are responsible for educating and referring troubled employees to available EAP services (BNA, 1987; Myers, 1984). Flight attendants are trained to detect and intervene in behavioral/medical problems of troubled flight attendants in order to help the employee obtain professional help. There are also programs offered directly by unions. By confronting troubled employees whose job performance has deteriorated, shop stewards function similarly to th The too-Numbelf several locations withir on). This program P1 to other treatment res senice, and increased ( the eye (BNA, i987)- and referral services (h in which several organ employees in the partic It should be noted not only to employees l affectan employee’s job (1986) found that 83% ffrilly members whose | Another manner it elllllioyee referral stratey model where an employ lirEAP is held in stri significant others (6 8 —fi— 54 function similarly to the supervisor in a company-sponsored program (Myers, 1984). The 800-Number is a telephone-based counseling service where employees from several locations within an organization can call in for assistance (BNA, 1987; Myers, 1984). This program provides for short-term counseling via listening along with referral to other treatment resources if needed. It has the advantages of low cost, 24-hour service, and increased comfort of clients who do not have to wait or look counselors in the eye (BNA, 1987). However, typically hot—lines offer minimal problem assessment and referral services (Myers, 1984). A final approach to be discussed is a consortium in which several organizations pool their resources and develop a program to serve employees in the participating organizations (Myers, 1984). It should be noted that in all the above approaches, EAP services may be available not only to employees but to their dependents as well whose personal problems could affect an employee’s job performance or personal well-being. However, while Straussner (1986) found that 83% of the EAPs were available to families, 39% counseled only family members whose problems were specifically related to an employee’s problem. Another manner in which EAPs exhibit diversity and flexibility is in the different employee referral strategies EAPs handle. These include: (1) a voluntary, self—referral model where an employee voluntarily seeks assistance, and his or her participation in the EAP is held in strict confidence; (2) a peer or significant others model where Significant others (e.g., family, coworkers) encourage the impaired employee to seek assistance (Wolf, 1982) and where participation in the EAP is held in strict confidence; and (3) a supervisory-referral, confrontation model where a third party within an organization (e.g., supervisor, union, medical department) actually refers an employee Whose performance is suffering to an EAP (Featherston & Bednarik, 1981; Fisher, 1983; Wrich,1980b). In this showed up and partiCi problem or treatment. Experts suggest th EAP users should be vr about the EAP is a ke Regardless of the educa refuse to acknowledge Supervisors can play a performance ratings ant get along with coworke Thus, supervisory-refer: Roman refer to the pr0( declining work perform tithe few legitimate ave life and motivate a that important, maintaining Performance is also criti a . rescuse for rnadequatr We also vary in l 55 Wrich, 1980b). In this case, supervisors are notified regarding whether the employee showed up and participated, but they are not advised of the nature of the employee’s problem or treatment. - Experts suggest that in order for programs to be considered successful, 40% of all EAP users should be voluntary, self-referrals (Hobson, 1982; Wrich, 1980b). Education about the EAP is a key component to obtaining support and usage from employees. Regardless of the education a company may provide, however, many employees will still refuse to acknowledge the existence of a problem and will fail to seek assistance. Supervisors can play a key role in breaking employees’ denial pattern by using job performance ratings and other documentation (e.g., absenteeism, irritability, inability to get along with coworkers) to identify troubled employees and refer them to an EAP. Thus, supervisory-referrals are considered important to EAP effectiveness. Trice and Roman refer to the process whereby a supervisor confronts an employee with his or her declining work performance in order to refer the worker to professional help as ”one of the few legitimate avenues, save police power," to effectively intervene in a worker’s life and motivate a change in behavior (1972, p. 171). Although confidentiality is important, maintaining the impaired employee’s accountability for his or her own job performance is also critical to successful intervention. Being treated cannot be used as an excuse for inadequate performance. EAPs also vary in the training and educational backgrounds of the staff. Staffing is often considered the most critical issue in an EAP (Masi, 1984). During the 1940’s when programs emphasized alcohol-related interventions, programs were typically staffed by recovering alcoholics. These programs relied a great deal on referrals from supervisors who were trained to detect and confront alcoholics. Today, EAP counselors are also responsible for dysfunctional work bei performance is impair employees about the | staffed by individuals we workers, psychiatrists, counselors, and person Finally, while EA appear to be consensus (1) Management (2) Union suppo (3) Assurance of (4) Written polic reSpousibilities of the or disc’p’inafy consequent "dining Programs; (5) ill health An employee i th . recognize s (6) A Ccess of serv 7 A suIiervisory iii ployee (e‘g's tardin. siveness, C) does audit, 56 are also responsible for training supervisors to detect ”troubled employees," to document dysfunctional work behaviors, and to recommend and/or refer employees to EAPs if job performance is impaired; i.e., supervisors are not expected to counsel or confront employees about the problem. Employee Assistance Program counselors today are staffed by individuals with diverse mental health backgrounds, which may include social workers, psychiatrists, psychologists, Alcoholics Anonymous members, nurses, trained counselors, and personnel employees (Masi, 1984). Finally, while EAPs vary in structure, models, and services provided, there does appear to be consensus on the basic components that encompass an effective EAP: (1) Management support; (2) Union support; (3) Assurance of confidentiality; (4) Written policies and procedures delineating the responsibilities of the company and employees regarding unacceptable work behaviors, disciplinary consequences, and methods of problem identification, such as supervisory training programs; (5) An employee education program to teach employees to take responsibility for their health, recognize symptoms, and refer themselves; (6) Access of services by dependents; (7) A supervisory training program which centers around identifying a troubled employee (e.g., tardiness, absenteeism, inability to get along with coworkers, defensiveness, etc.), documenting deteriorating performance, confrontational methods, and the referral process; (8) Breadth of counseling and clinical services which provide easy and convenient access and include dia (9) Labor and n (10) Health insur (11) Adequate cc of the EAP to empl presentations, etc); am (12) Professional iGroeneveld, 1980). While the above assessment and referral EAP and that the sta alilirrrpriately. There has been a IT lithe faStgTOWth of the: P brains have not been lilan . agcmem, unions) sir 57 access and include diagnosis, treatment, referral, follow—up and evaluation; (9) Labor and management orientation and labor-steward training; (10) Health insurance coverage; (11) Adequate communication of the existence, objectives, functions and services of the EAP to employees (via newsletters, brochures, payroll stuffers, posters, presentations, etc.); and (12) Professional leadership (Dickman & Emener, 1982; Greenwood, 1983; Shain & Groeneveld, 1980). While the above are all desirable components, Wrich (1982) indicates that assessment and referral are the critical links between the troubled employee and the EAP and that the staff specialist/counselor must be trained to assess and refer appropriately. EAP Research There has been a minimal amount of empirical research related to EAPs compared to the fast growth of these programs (Jones, 1983; Roman, 1984). Employee Assistance Programs have not been required to report their activities, except to their sponsors (e.g., management, unions) since they are not subject to any voluntary or governmental agency regulations or accountability requirements, as are mental health and drug and alcohol abuse programs (Jones, 1983). Roman,(1984) characterized the current state of EAP research as "a hodgepodge of materials which have been prepared by persons whose credentials range from experienced social scientists to cynical self-promoters, all of which comprise a shapeless bag of ’findings’ and ’data’." In addition, much research that does exist focuses primarily or is intertwined with research on OAPs (Fielding, 1984a). While these narrower. In a review of alcv “most studies indicate here isa 10:1 payback and improved product measures used in OAF changes in drinking bel absenteeism, illness, a absenteeism, job efficir Program reaches a targr of the cases examined. concerns of validity EDP brief follow-up times 5 treatment staff). Most of the literat or uses on characteristi seo ° pa and administratii 58 1984a). While these programs were the forerunners of vE-APs, their focus was much narrower. In a review of alcoholism programs, Tersine and Hazeldine (1982, p. 72) stated that "most studies indicate a success rate of 50 to 85%." They further cite an estimate that there is a 10:1 payback from alcoholism programs due to less turnover and absenteeism and improved productivity. In a review of OAPs by Kurtz et a1. (1984), a variety of measures used in OAP evaluations were found, which were grouped into four classes: changes in drinking behavior (abstinence, rehabilitation), work performance level (e.g., absenteeism, illness, accidents, turnover, efficiency), cost reduction (savings from absenteeism, job efficiency), and penetration rates of risk groups (i.e., the extent a program reaches a target population). Favorable outcomes were reported in over 60% of the cases examined, although also noted was the lack of rigorous standards and concerns of validity applied to the evaluation processes (e.g., lack of control groups, too- brief follow-up times, subject selection problems, poor employer documentation and treatment staff). Most of the literature directly examining EAPs dates from the early 1970’s and focuses on characteristics of the EAPs themselves, company practices related to the scope and administration of such programs, and provides primarily descriptions of individual programs and anecdotal reports (Akabas et al., 1979; Bierman, 1981; Bloom, 1986; BNA, 1974; Googins, 1984; Gould & McKenzie, 1984; Masi, 1979; Miller, 1977; Rivera, 1984; Skidmore etal., 1974; Smirnow, 1980; Straussner, 1986; Weissman, 1976), surveys related to EAPs (Erfurt & F oote, 1977; Ford & McLaughlin, 1981; Grimes, 1980; Opinion Research Corporation, 1972, 1974, 1976, 1979), and current reviews of the field (Leavitt, 1983; Roman, 1981). A major area of effectiveness of the prv and "outcome." Rese; section on EAP benef understanding of the fr the following section i factors found to influe Research on EAP Util Terms which his ”participation " utilization/participatior refers to the actual usag of troubled employees ' I . . can rn-person vrsit to referral. When the tern ll - art' ' t rcrpatron" or "usa h ave personal or work-r lieEAP "utilization ra 59 A major area of EAP research which companies are interested in focuses on the effectiveness of the programs. Effectiveness also refers to program "success," "impact," and "outcome." Research on EAP effectiveness was reported in the first chapter in the section on EAP benefits. Since the purpose of the present research is to gain a better understanding of the factors influencing one’s decision to seek assistance from an EAP, the following section will present EAP research on utilization/participation rates and factors found to influence EAP utilization/participation. Research on EAP Utilization/Participation Terms which have been used synonymously with EAP "utilization” include "participation" and ”penetration”, particularly when referring to utilization/participation/penetration rates. "Employee Assistance Program utilization" refers to the actual usage of an EAP by an employee, or the total number or percentage of troubled employees who participate in EAP services. The term "EAP usage" refers to an in-person visit to the EAP office or a call for an appointment, information, or a referral. When the term "utilization" is used in this paper, it will also refer to the term "participation" or "usage” in an EAP. Utilization does not refer to employees who may have personal or work-related problems but have chosen not to participate in an EAP. The EAP ”utilization rate” typically refers to the ratio of EAP clients to the employee population (Myers, 1984), which is similar to the "penetration rate"--or the rate at which employees have penetrated the EAP. If the reported EAP success rates of 60-90% (Baxter, 1981; Holden, 1973; Judd, 1980; Shetty, 1982; United States Congress, 1982) are to have meaning, we have to assume that a significant percentage of troubled employees are participating in the prom, Therefore, in its evaluation pr0< discussing utilization r is the fact that a ”gree keep systematic data I For example, in records on utilization 1 average rate of 13%. utilization rates cited- Champion, 1988; Dunl Groeneveld, 1980; Stra clout-year period (Uri cited by specific EAPs Steelcase, Ina-20%, U etal.,1977). Since the rtwould be extremely us In hrs research on 60 program. Therefore, in order for any program to be judged successful, one component in its evaluation process should include employee utilization. Unfortunately, when discussing utilization rates, one major problem found in an EAP study by Weiss (1980) is the fact that a "great many" of the responding companies who had an EAP did not keep systematic data regarding usage of their program. For example, in an examination of 14 EAPs by Uyeno (1988), nine maintained records on utilization rates--which ranged from a low of 1.1% to a high of 36%, with an average rate of 13%. In other research, there have been a variety of estimates on utilization rates cited-—ranging from lows of 1% to 5% (Busch, 1981; Cahill, 1983; Champion, 1988; Dunkin, 1981; Featherston & Bednarek, 1981; Marino, 1985; Shain & Groeneveld, 1980; Straussner, 1986), to a 7% utilization rate at General Motors during a four-year period (United States Congress, 1982), to reports ranging from 9% to 30% cited by specific EAPs; e.g., Boston College--9%, Michigan State University-~3-14%, Steelcase, Inc.—-20%, University of Missouri--30% (Grimes 1980; Masi, 1978; Thoresen et al., 1977). Since the range of reported utilization rates is extremely broad, it seems it would be extremely useful to better understand factors which might influence this rate. In his research on EAPs, Donald Jones (1983) developed benchmarks or standards on which to compare programs. One of these standards was the percentage of employees who should utilize a company’s EAP-~which was estimated to be between 5- 10% after the first year of operation. Some question this figure since this utilization rate was based on an externally-contracted EAP that served many organizations. Also, while Kurtz, Googins and Howard (1984) examined penetration or utilization rates of risk gr OUps when evaluating occupational programs, Herring (1987) suggests that what constitutes a "successful" penetration rate is not known and that not all populations are equally penetrable. Regardless of ii programs and their re they are designed to as never receive treatmei of Health, Education, the programs fall far sh are seeing only about 4 1987, p. 140). If it is personal problems, thi: rot obtaining the assist Avariety of varia research. One approa have used EAP service client statistics regardin raceoccupational level stress, financial probler Significant other-refers inconsistent. Research f Problem type, and refer W any researchers ha m . arrred (Cromidas 19E 61 equally penetrable. Regardless of the standard and despite the need for occupational assistance programs and their reported effectiveness, EAPs often remain underutilized by those they are designed to assist. For example, it has been estimated that 85% of all alcoholics never receive treatment (Matsunaga, 1983). Joseph Califano, J r., the former Secretary of Health, Education, and Welfare, argues that deSpite the successes reported by EAPs, the programs fall far short of their potential. He adds that “even the ’effective’ programs are seeing only about 4 percent of the problem drinkers among their employees." (BNA, 1987, p. 140). If it is true that up to 30% of the U.S. workforce may have serious personal problems, this would suggest EAPs are underutilized and many employees are not obtaining the assistance that could improve their work and personal lives. A variety of variables and approaches have been represented in EAP utilization research. One approach consists of researchers examining profiles of employees who have used EAP services. This research, however, has focused primarily on examining client statistics regarding participation by demographics (e.g., age, gender, marital status, race, occupational level, education, income, tenure), problem type (e.g., substance abuse, stress, financial problems), and referral source (i.e., self-referral, supervisory-referral, significant other-referral). In addition, much of the research on utilization is often inconsistent. Research findings regarding EAP utilization/participation by demographics, problem type, and referral source are reported below. Marianna: Many researchers have found that the majority of individuals utilizing the EAP are married (Cromidas, 1987; Marino, 1985; Uyeno, 1988) with the second most common user being divorced ( in spouses encouragi employees may not fe other problem areas . substance abuse, fmar Gender Research findinj researchers have conc more than males (Cha 1988), others have four. 1986), while still other females (Grimes, 1980. Research has also again the results are i females use services Int 1984) while others to “fluently and female iStraussner, 1986). 62 user being divorced (Uyeno, 1988). In general, it is assumed that being married results in spouses encouraging one another to seek assistance (Serxner, 1988). While single employees may not feel the need to use an EAP for marital or child—rearing problems, other problem areas are still endemic to this group which the EAP could assist (e.g., substance abuse, financial problems). Gender. Research findings on utilization rates and gender are mixed. While many researchers have concluded that females utilize EAP and occupational health services more than males (Champion, 1988; Hung, 1988; Jorrisch, 1986; Serxner, 1988; Uyeno, 1988), others have found that males tend to use the services more frequently (DeFuentes, 1986), while still other researchers conclude utilization rates are equal among males and females (Grimes, 1980; Straussner, 1986; Sudduth, 1984). Research has also been conducted on EAP usage by gender and program type, but again the results are inconclusive. For example, some researchers have found that females use services more if the program is on—site/internal (Champion, 1988; Sudduth, 1984) while others concluded that males tend to use on-site/internal EAPs more frequently and females tend to make more use of off-site/external contractors (Straussner, 1986). Race M ) Race is another demographic characteristic examined with regard to utilization, and as with gender, conclusions are often mixed. Regarding general health behavior, Blacks have been found to have fewer physician visits and telephone consultations than Whites p- but twice the rate 1 However, others have Hung, 1988; Jorrisch, of health services in group seems to depenr (1988) and Hung ( I98: Champion (1988) fou Mexican-Americans h to the presence of inft as cited in Serxner, 19 Usage by race anr internal/on-site and er tend to use external, or internal, in-house prof bgg The age of most research conducted. Ir ages of 20~29 with the s (1988) reported an ave users ranged from 18 it ——— 63 but twice the rate of hospital outpatient utilization than Whites (Serxner, 1988). However, others have found that Whites are the primary EAP users (Champion, 1988; Hung, 1988; Jorrisch, 1986; Uyeno, 1988; Straussner, 1986) as well as the primary users of health services in general (Rosenstock & Kirscht, 1979). The second major user group seems to depend on the particular research conducted. For example, while Uyeno (1988) and Hung ( 1988) found Hispanics were the second major user group after Whites, Champion (1988) found that Blacks were second with Hispanics third. In general, Mexican-Americans have been found to underuse mental health services, due probably to the presence of informal family support discouraging such use (Keefe & Casas, 1980 as cited in Serxner, 1988). Usage by race and EAP type has also been examined. Straussner (1986) compared internal/on-site and external/off—site usage rates and concluded that White employees tend to use external, contracted—out programs more often while minority employees use internal, in-house programs more frequently. has The age of most EAP users also appears to vary depending on the particular research conducted. In research by Uyeno (1988), most EAP users were between the ages of 20-29 with the second most common age group between 40-49. However, Hung (1988) reported an average age of 38.6, while DeFuentes (1986) found that the age of users ranged from 18 to over 60 with the majority in the 26-44 age group. Why Leavitt (1983) found that employees with more organizational seniority utilize the EAP services more frequently, as was shown in a 1975 study at Oldsmobile where the average seniority was study of four firms w Hung(1988) also fou; EAP. When seniority employees with greats off-site programs. management support MW DeFuentes (198i salaried employees use that lower occupatior employees, are the higl 1986; Suddeth, 1984; erIrployees utilized EA and executive (3.5%) er of executives and man: pork related problems at toplevel manager: —7— 64 average seniority was ten years. A similar result was found three years later in a 1978 study of four firms where tenure of users averaged from 8 to 18 years (Leavitt, 1983). Hung (1988) also found the greater one’s seniority, the greater the willingness to use an EAP. When seniority was examined by program type, Sudduth (1984) found that employees with greater seniority utilized internal, on-site programs more than external, off-site programs. Sudduth suggested that external programs may receive less management support so they are utilized less. Occupational Level DeFuentes (1986) and Roman (1984) suggested professionals, supervisors and salaried employees use EAPs more often. However, there appears to be greater support that lower occupational levels, such as the office/technical group and hourly-paid employees, are the highest EAP users (Hung, 1988; Ford & McLaughlin, 1981; Jorrisch, 1986; Suddeth, 1984; Uyeno, 1988). For example, Uyeno (1988) found blue collar employees utilized EAPs more frequently (50.2%) compared with white collar (46.1%) and executive (3.5%) employees. Other studies have identified the under-representation of executives and managers who tend to be reluctant to seek help for work and non- work related problems (NIAAA, 1982; Trice & Beyer, 1980). Leavitt (1983) suggested that top-level managers tend to use EAP services less frequently because they are less subject to coercion from the employer to seek assistance. Other reasons cited for managerial under-utilization include the lack of supervision which makes performance problems less visible (Myers & Myers, 1985) and the fear of jeopardizing future career Opportunities (Hung, 1988). It often takes a serious event such as an automobile accident, heart attack denial even then is ‘ (Johnson, 1973). The type of EA relationship between t examined occupation: site, contracted-out p under-represented by reflected the status of Baxter (1976) ex. EAP usage by family Period, the majority r members (22%), facul hm in a review of dar band that lower incon was the middle income I . sweat and highest inco Kirscht (1979) also for t efluently, while those more frequently 0a the other hanr us its. Hung reported 1 65 accident, heart attack, or emotional breakdown for a problem to be acknowledged, and denial even then is very strong without additional influence from significant others (Johnson, 1973). The type of EAP model or program (internal or external) may moderate the relationship between occupational level and EAP usage. For example, Straussner (1986) examined occupational level and program type. In that research, it was found that off— site, contracted-out programs were over—represented by higher—level employees and under-represented by lower-level workers while internal, on-sitc programs better reflected the status of the workforce (Straussner, 1986). Baxter (1976) examined the organizational level of employees and the degree of EAP usage by family members at Rutgers University and found that in a two-year period, the majority of the users were at the general staff level (49%), then family members (22%), faculty (18%), and administrative and professional workers (10%). became In a review of data prior to 1965 on income and utilization rates, Serxner (1988) found that lower income employees used health services the least; however, by 1968 it was the middle income group which underutilized physicians and health care while the lowest and highest income groups used health services most frequently. Rosenstock and Kirscht (1979) also found that those below poverty level used medical services less frequently, while those in the higher socioeconomic groups tend to use health services more frequently. On the other hand, Hung (1988) reached the opposite conclusion regarding EAP usage. Hung reported that in his examination of EAPs, the highest-income employees were the lowest EAP was the second lowesr and 32030000) utili: income group are als to agree with researc services the least. Emafl There was not or 0988) examined this the future if needed a 10 use an EAP in the —7 66 were the lowest EAP users ($50-69,000) and the lowest-income group (less than $30,000) was the second lowest user group. The two middle income employee groups ($30-39,000 and $20-30,000) utilized the services most frequently. If we assume those in the highest income group are also in the higher occupational levels, then Hung’s research appears to agree with research on occupational levels where executives were found to use the services the least. Education There was not much available literature found on education and EAP usage. Hung (1988) examined this variable as it related to employees’ willingness to use an EAP in the future if needed and found that those who were more educated were more willing to use an EAP in the future. Problem Type and EAP Utilization/Participation The preceding sections focused on demographic factors influencing EAP utilization. This section will examine research on utilization and the types of problems employees/clients seek assistance on at the EAP. These findings also tend to be mixed. Champion (1988) and Wells (1988) found marital, family, child, and psychological problems were the main ones handled at EAPs. However, Savoca (1986) and Baxter (1976) found EAP clients sought help mainly for job—related, emotional, and drinking problems. Others agreed in part with each of the above researchers. For example, Cromidas (1987), Jorrisch (1986), and Straussner (1986) found the primary problems presented at an EAP were for emotional, chemical dependence, and marital problems. DeFuentes (1986) found the primary problems encountered were for alcohol, legal, and emotional problems. along with poor i0 encountered at the l DeFuentes (191 presented at the EA] to addition, males, \ substance abuse prol emotional problems ( Bloom (1986) ex employees to an EAP ref errals: tardiness/at personal problems, en Referral Source nd E 67 emotional problems. Substance abuse, financial, legal, emotional, and marital problems along with poor job performance and absenteeism were the primary problems encountered at the 14 EAPs examined by Uyeno (1988). DeFuentes (1986) examined gender and ethnicity and the type of problem presented at the EAP. Both males and females equally experienced family problems. In addition, males, Whites, and Hispanics most frequently experienced alcohol and substance abuse problems while females and Blacks sought assistance primarily on emotional problems (DeFuentes, 1986). Bloom (1986) examined the primary types of problems which supervisors referred employees to an EAP for and found the following were frequently cited as reasons for referrals: tardiness/absenteeism, deteriorating work performance, preoccupation with personal problems, emotional problems, and suspected alcoholism and drug abuse. Referral Source and EAP Utilization/Participation The last area of research on EAP utilization to be discussed focuses on EAP utilization and referral source. There are three basic referral approaches to EAP utilization: self-referral, supervisory referral, and referral by significant others. Self- referral or "voluntarism" has been shown to be a key factor influencing the success of an EAP (Wrich, 1974). Supervisory referrals are another major source since deterioration of an employee’s job performance is one of the basic reasons referrals are made to an EAP. Supervisors are able to get employees whose work performance has deteriorated to seek assistance because supervisors can threaten them with disciplinary action (Wrich, 1974). Significant others, defined as "those persons who exercise major influence over the attitudes and behavior of individuals" (Woelfel & Haller, 1971, p. 75), is the last major referral in general, self-refe: seek assistance at an EA last main source (81001 Straussner,1986; UyenO, Uyeno (1988) found all p referrals and the second next. Similarly, Bloom (I examined were self~referr examination of 32 EAPs i; Stpervisory and peer (sigr ERR clients interviewed s 65% were referred by suj: it. Finally, while Straussnr 77' 68 is the last major referral method and includes referral by peers, coworkers, family, etc. In general, self—referrals are the most common source by which employees/clients seek assistance at an EAP, with supervisory referrals second and significant others the last main source (Bloom, 1986; Coleman, 1984; DeFuentes, 1986; Pardue, 1987; Straussner, 1986; Uyeno, 1988). For example, in a study of 14 companies with EAPs, Uyeno (1988) found all programs examined reported the majority of referrals were self- referrals and the second major source came from supervisors, with significant others next. Similarly, Bloom (1986) found that approximately 80% of the clients in the EAPs examined were self-referred and 20% were supervisory referred. In Coleman’s (1984) examination of 32 EAPs in higher education, most users were self-referred, followed by supervisory and peer (significant others) referred. Pardue (1987) found 79% of the 200 EAP clients interviewed stated they realized the need for assistance on their own while 6.5% were referred by supervisors and 6.5% came because a fellow employee suggested it. Finally, while Straussner (1986) found self-referrals (54%) and supervisory—referrals (25%) were the main referral sources, medical (13%) and other referrals by union, family members, etc. (8%) were secondary sources. In a look at gender and referral type, DeFuentes (1986) found that both male and female users were primarily self-referred (52.4%), followed by males and females who were referred by the medical department (16.9%), the supervisor (14.7%) and the union (8%). Several explanations have been provided to help us understand why employees seek assistance at an EAP. In describing the referral process, most research has focused either on the self-referral (voluntary) process or the supervisory-referral process. The major referral sources (self, supervisory, significant others), however, are not completely independent of one an0 may also equally influenr simultaneously recommr Trice (1978), Myers (19' process as being influenr (1) a family membe and influences an emplo referrals by significant or (2) financial conside from seeking help; (3) the degree to w insistence on the employ: (4) the amount of p and its services and about provided to help employg (5) the existence of a ”“1113ng voluntary pa. (6) to AP tenure sin b rcause employees’ belief 69 independent of one another since many of the factors influencing one type of referral may also equally influence another type. In addition, more than one referral source may simultaneously recommend/pressure an employee to seek EAP assistance. Beyer and Trice (1978), Myers (1984), Scanlon (1986), and Wrich (1974) describe the referral process as being influenced by the following factors: (1) a family member, a friend, or a coworker seeks information regarding the EAP and influences an employee to seek help~-this factor may affect both self-referrals and referrals by significant others; (2) financial considerations regarding the cost of services may discourage employees from seeking help; (3) the degree to which the employee feels free of hassle from the EAP staff’s insistence on the employee modifying his or her behavior; (4) the amount of publicity and education received by employees about the EAP and its services and about particular problems (i.e., the amount of self—analysis literature provided to help employees recognize behavioral patterns of a troubled employee); (5) the existence of a supportive climate on the part of the company and union in encouraging voluntary participation; and (6) EAP tenure since new EAPs must overcome initial employee skepticism because employees’ belief in program effectiveness is necessary to gain credibility. A number of additional reasons affecting EAP utilization by employees have been SUggested in the literature. Some of these include: (7) fear of a breach of confidentiality (Myers, 1984; Myers & Myers, 1985; Wrich, 1974); (8) the perception of a possible adverse affect on tenure or promotional opportunities (Myers, 1‘ (9) fear of a social an alcoholic, a drug addi dRoman, 1978); (10) the degree of cr (1]) lack of a with (12) type 0f progr Scanlon, 1986); (13) EAP model tyy (14) the occupation: 1981; Hung, 1988; JorriSr Peter, 1980; Uyeno, 19m (15) fear of giving t1 (Hung, 1988). In related research 1 torrrfluence an individuai sPtrrptoms. hese factors identified include: (1) 6 the perceived u on (18) (19 ran). availability of a. the costs of the i the general valor From the above list, 70 opportunities (Myers, 1984; Myers & Myers, 1985; Wrich, 1974); (9) fear of a social stigma attached to EAP utilization (e.g., label person as being an alcoholic, a drug addict, in a troubled marriage, etc.) (Brewer & McAvoy, 1986; Trice & Roman, 1978); (10) the degree of convenience and accessibility of the EAP services (Wrich, 1980a); (11) lack of a written policy statement defining responsibilities (Wrich, 1980a); (12) type of program sponsorship (union, management, joint) (Roman, 1984; Scanlon, 1986); (13) EAP model type (internal/on-site vs. external/off—site) (Straussner, 1986); (14) the occupational level of the employee (DeFuentes, 1986; Ford & McLaughlin, 1981; Hung, 1988; Jorrisch, 1986; Leavitt, 1983; Roman, 1984; Suddeth, 1984; Trice & Beyer, 1980; Uyeno, 1988); and (15) fear of giving the impression of being unable to manage one’s own problems (Hung, 1988). In related research by Rosenstock and Kirscht (1979), several factors were found to influence an individual’s decision to seek professional help in the presence of illness symptoms. These factors may also have an effect on seeking EAP assistance. Factors identified include: (16) the perceived urgency and severity of the condition; (17) availability of alternative paths of action open to individuals; (18) the costs of the different courses of action; and (19) the general value an individual places on medical care (Rosenstock & Kirscht, 1979). From the above list, it may be concluded that the employee’s perception of the utility of utilizing EAP se the employee has a pro affected by the perceptio As previously mer important means of enco areina good position of i mightbenefit from EAP I however, often resist bet which have been found It (1) there usually ha Supervisor will confront a (2) often ambivale. ”Wilfrid employee so bot (3) nrany supervisor: a - . retequrred in order to c? (4) organizations diff behaviors (Molloy, 1986) I 3 (5) the type of EAP (residuum, 1984, cont intern alprogranrscomparr iilajo ' "‘Slie EAP S are 11] management) . WW ( 1980b) adds pro cess for the following i 71 utility of utilizing EAP services is a key factor influencing his/her behavior. Even though the employee has a problem, the individual’s decision to participate in an EAP is affected by the perception of the positive consequences of such a decision. As previously mentioned, supervisory referrals have been recognized as an important means of encouraging employees to utilize EAP services because supervisors are in a good position of identifying “unsatisfactory job performance" of employees who might benefit from EAP participation (Gam, Sauser, Evans, & Lair, 1983). Supervisors, however, often resist becoming involved in a formal referral process. Some factors which have been found to influence supervisory referrals include the following: (1) there usually has to be sufficient deterioration in job performance before a supervisor will confront and refer an employee (Wrich, 1980a); (2) often ambivalence permeates the relationship between a supervisor and a troubled employee so both parties frequently utilize cover—up strategies (Myers, 1984); (3) many supervisors have failed to develop "good” human relations skills which are required in order to deal with troubled employees (Googins & Kurtz, 1980); (4) organizations differ in the extent supervisors are required to monitor workers’ behaviors (Molloy, 1986); (5) the type of EAP model (on-site vs. off-site) influences referrals by supervisors (e.g., Sudduth, 1984, concluded that there were more supervisory referrals to on-site, internal programs compared with external programs, presumably because supervisors feel that on—site EAPs are more integrated into the organization and accepted by top management). Roman (1980b) adds that supervisors fail to become involved in an EAP referral process for the following reasons: (b) supervisors expt (7) supervisors fail ' (8) supervisors do r (9) supervisors are a and (10) supervisors don’t stigma that may be attack Stump This chapter prOVidf in the United States. BUS troubled employees over Taylor in the early 1900 tcgatively influenced the I tried to ignore the person: SPacific duties in exchang We 10 problems outside I shaPe up or ship out (BN. 3P the 1940’s and 19 72 (6) supervisors expect the dysfunctional behaviors will disappear; (7) supervisors fail to see an EAP as a legitimate organizational strategy; (8) supervisors do not want to be involved with employees; (9) supervisors are afraid they will lose power over employees after they refer them; and (10) supervisors don’t want to label or mis-label an employee or be involved in any stigma that may be attached to the employee once referred. Summary This chapter provided an historical overview of organizational assistance programs in the United States. Businesses have changed their perspective and treatment toward troubled employees over the years. Initially, the scientific management attitudes of Taylor in the early 1900’s, with the emphasis solely on efficiency and productivity, negatively influenced the treatment and attitude toward problem employees. Employers tried to ignore the personal problems of employees and expected employees to perform specific duties in exchange for wages. If an employee’s performance at work suffered due to problems outside the control of managers, then the employee was expected to shape up or ship out (BNA, 1987; Wrich, 1980b). By the 1940’s and 1950’s, however, a concern about the alcohol-related behaviors of employees evolved and precursors of Employee Assistance Programs (EAPs) developed in the form of Occupational Alcoholism Programs (OAPs). Occupational Alcoholism Programs were considered an innovation in resolving job performance problems and mental—health issues by attempting to help alcoholics in the workplace. The success of these programs was typically based on the employee’s return to an acceptable level 0f l0b l expansion of occupation legal, medical and other brush EAPs. Organizations now long-run than if their p personal, social, emotion illnesses, absenteeism, ac 1984b). Human resour affecting troubled empl (Scanlon,l983). As EAI it the workplace has alsv fliers employees whose tissrstance Programs toe Promote voluntary/self-re EAPs vary in both pro gram format, services to ' ntrnued to evolve over acti ' ' vrty make it necessary retaini ‘ og their human re prov' ‘ rde assrstance to em] aforties. While EAPs have be if add ' ressrng both work 73 acceptable level of job performance. By the 1970’s, early OAP successes led to the expansion of occupational programs to focus on a broader range of personal, social, legal, medical and other problems that hinder job performance--known today as broad— brush EAPs. Organizations now recognize that untreated troubled employees cost more in the long-run than if their problems were diagnosed and treated early. Employees with personal, social, emotional, and other problems are less productive, have higher rates of illnesses, absenteeism, accidents, and disability than those who are ”healthy" (Fielding, 1984b). Human resource managers have realized that by attacking the problems affecting troubled employees, the corporation can experience measurable savings (Scanlon, 1983). As EAPs have replaced alcohol-based programs, the supervisor’s role in the workplace has also changed. The supervisor is now perceived as an agent who refers employees whose performance has deteriorated to specialists. Employee Assistance Programs today advocate not only supervisory referrals but attempt to promote voluntary/self—referrals as well. EAPs vary in both management philosophy and structural dimensions (e.g., program format, services provided, evaluation methods) (Myers, 1984). They have continued to evolve over time as employee needs, foreign competition, and legislative activity make it necessary for employers to take a pro-active stance in maintaining and retaining their human resources. Employee Assistance Programs have the ability to provide assistance to employees/clients who have not traditionally used social service agencies. While EAPs have become an integral part of American business and are capable of addressing both work and nonwork-related problems of employees through a comprehensive system 0' they are most designer identification increases I (Busch, 1981; Father, Progression of substance- intervention and treatmE benefit-through refill“ performance, and impr C Wrich, 1980b). While descriptive re has been conducted along more research is requirer use EAP services. Reset utilization. However, mu users and lacks a theor underlying factors influer 0"What the employee 1 Understanding why the be The variables of inte rtsearch have been conce selected as a guiding ham or " ' ‘ rndrvrdual’s decrsron~ or ' ervrew of control the aPProaches found in the 74 comprehensive system of services, EAPs often remain underutilized by those employees they are most designed to reach. Most professionals agree that early problem identification increases treatment success and is necessary to obtain maximum results (Busch, 1981; Farber, 1982; Ford & McLaughlin, 1981; Witte & Cannon, 1979). Progression of substance-abuse and other personal problems may then be avoided. Early intervention and treatment are important because both the employee and the employer benefit—-through reduced absenteeism, sick time and accidents, improved job performance, and improved work and spousal relationships (Googins & Kurtz, 1980; Wrich, 1980b). While descriptive research on EAP models, services provided, and clients of EAPs has been conducted along with empirical research on the effectiveness of these programs, more research is required to examine factors influencing why individuals use or do not use EAP services. Researchers have presented a multitude of factors that affect EAP utilization. However, much of the present research emphasizes demographic profiles of users and lacks a theoretical base. Therefore, there is a need to understand the underlying factors influencing the EAP utilization process. Researchers have focused on what the employee has done (i.e., used or not used EAP services) but not on understanding why the behavior occurred. The variables of interest in examining factors influencing EAP usage in the present research have been conceptualized in a control theory framework. Control theory was selected as a guiding framework because its underlying premise attempts to understand an individual’s decision-making behavior. The following chapter will provide an overview of control theory and its relation to organizational health models and approaches found in the literature. The model developed in the present research to examine factors influenc will then be presented a 75 examinefactors influencing the process involved in the decision to utilize EAP services will then be presented along with the hypotheses of interest. CONTROL 1 Meant As previously state individual’s decision-m general systems theory I that utilizes the basic disconnection of feedb framework is based on t one must understand the systems perspective. While control thee acceptance in such diver and medicine until Wien 1982). The theory has al individuals due to the we Carver and Scheier (1982 heselfregulation of bus and social psychology, 9 1982 fora review) and 0 j . 939, Hollenbeck & Brie ”West 1987;Tayroi, Fis In general, control t CHAPTER 3 CONTROL THEORY AND A MODEL OF EAP UTILIZATION Control Theory As previously stated, control theory has been used to explain factors influencing an individual’s decision-making behavior. Control theory is based on a cybernetic or general systems theory perspective. General systems theory is a meta-theory or strategy that utilizes the basic concepts of feedback, self-regulation, and disregulation--i.e., disconnection of feedback loops among the system’s parts (Schwartz, 1979). The framework is based on the view that to understand the behavior of a system as a whole, one must understand the interaction of its parts. Control theory is based on this general systems perspective. While control theory has been around for a long time, it did not gain wide acceptance in such diverse disciplines as engineering, applied mathematics, economics, and medicine until Wiener (1948) published his book on cybernetics (Carver & Scheier, 1982). The theory has also been applied in the fields of psychology and the behavior of individuals due to the work of Miller, Galanter, and Pribram (1960), Powers (1973), and Carver and Scheier (1982). For example, many theorists have applied control theory to the self~regulation of human behavior in the fields of cognitive psychology, personality and social psychology, clinical psychology, health psychology (see Carver & Scheier, 1982 for a review) and organizational behavior (Campion & Lord, 1982; Hollenbeck, 1989; Hollenbeck & Brief, 1987; Hollenbeck & Williams, 1987; Klein, 1989; Lord & Hanges, 1987; Taylor, Fisher & llgen, 1984). In general, control theory deals with the manner in which systems (e.g., humans) 76 collect and process in: (Campion & Lord, 1981 emphasizes goals (also behavior by focusing on is a difference between i an individual is drinking adiscrepancy exists bet discrepancy which is if individual to respond in Control theory of f (TOTE) cybernetics pri called the feedback loo detects discrepancies be monitoring process of g (also called the goal, st iscrepartcy is perceives activity to reduce the d 77 collect and process information in order to achieve and maintain a desired state (Campion & Lord, 1981; Carver & Scheier, 1981; Lord & Hanges, 1987). Control theory emphasizes goals (also called standards or referent values) and feedback to regulate behavior by focusing on goal-feedback discrepancies. A discrepancy exists when there is a difference between what is desired and what one currently has. For example, when an individual is drinking more than desired or is experiencing more stress than desired, a discrepancy exists between the desired and current state. It is the detection of a discrepancy which is the basis for action because a discrepancy may motivate an individual to respond in some way in order to reduce the discrepancy. Control theory of human motivation is an elaboration of the test-operate—test—exit (TOTE) cybernetics principle proposed by Miller et al., (1960). Miller et a1. (1960) called the feedback loop the TOTE sequence. The TOTE sequence (see Figure 4) detects discrepancies between the current and desired states by engaging in a self- monitoring process of Legging input data on the current state against the desired state (also called the goal, standard, or referent value). According to the theory, when a discrepancy is perceived, the system (e.g., the individual) initiates (operates) some activity to reduce the discrepancy. The test-operate sequence is repeated until the discrepancy is perceived to be eliminated or reduced to an acceptable level-—at which time the sequence is terminated via an ex_it process. An example illustrating this process is when an individual monitors his/her current body temperature (for example, using a thermometer) and compares it to the desired temperature or goal (usually 98.6 degrees). If a discrepancy is found between the desired and current states, then the individual attempts to reduce the discrepancy through some response (e.g., taking aspirin, going to a doctor). The monitoring process continues until the temperature returns to the (137001 Source: Ad0pt Figure 4 z The 78 >' Te 8 t ' ,9- (Congrats? (fizcongrzzit‘y) a, 0;) era z‘e Source: Adopted from Miller, Galanter & Pribram, 1960 Figure 4: The TOTE Unit individual’s desired ten Other models has discrepancy-reducing p models include additior understand the decision below are Carver and Sr Carver and Scheie Their illustration of the elements: (1) a refers input/sensor/perception fraction. A fifth eleme. impacts on the four m feedback 100p is an 0Pe Three of the elements or comparator, and the out; the loop along with the it. further divided into cogt in the Carver and . st andards or objectives ' 79 individual’s desired temperature (goal). Other models have been developed by control theorists which are based on a discrepancy-reducing process similar to that proposed by Miller et al. (1960). These models include additional elements and are typically more detailed to help us better understand the decision-making process. Two useful models of control theory discussed below are Carver and Scheier’s (1982) and Campion and Lord’s (1982) models. Carver and Scheier’s (1982) model applied control theory to human behavior. Their illustration of the negative feedback loop in Figure 5 consists of four main elements: (1) a referent value which is also called a standard or goal, (2) an input/sensor/perception function, (3) a comparator, and (4) an output or effector function. A fifth element known as "disturbance” is the external environment which impacts on the four main system elements. Disturbance is included because the feedback loop is an open system and is influenced by external environmental forces. Three of the elements originate within the loop: the input or perception function, the comparator, and the output function. The standard or reference value originates outside the 100p along with the "disturbance" (Carver & Scheier, 1982). These elements can be further divided into cognitive and affective components as described below. In the Carver and Scheier (1982) model, goals may be either explicit or implicit standards or objectives. The input function senses the present state or condition, which is then compared. against the goal/standard through the comparator mechanism. The goals and the process of matching inputs to standards encompass the cognitive component of the model. According to the model, if a discrepancy is detected, negative affect may occur (the affective component) which may be in the form of dissatisfaction, frustration, or anger (Carver & Scheier, 1981). Based on the model, it is this negative (PE SOUrCe Figure 80 REFERENCE VALUE if t COMPARATOR INPUT ' ourpur Funcrrou . surrender (assessment 1- (BEHAVTOR) runner on i [ suvrnormsarr t—Forsruaaaucs Source: Carver & Scheier, 1982 Figure 5: The Negative Feedback Loop affect which motivates : behavioral output fun discrepancy directly, environment. This imp which leads to a differe isthe perception of the discrepancy, rather tha and responding contin provides a dynamic per A feedback loop where a system (e.g., i current and desired stat discrePoncy between the sensing Ola disCrepanc regulate human motivati information on the cur u . . ndesrrable Ill mechanic is often not necessarily Scheier (1981) those 81 affect which motivates an individual to initiate a discrepancy-reducing response, or some behavioral output function. The behavioral output is not expected to reduce the discrepancy directly, but rather it does so through its impact on the system’s environment. This impact is then hypothesized to create a change in the present state, which leads to a different perception, which is then again compared to the standard. It is the perception of the current compared to the desired state that results in a perceived discrepancy, rather than an objective state. The entire process of sensing, comparing, and responding continues until the discrepancy disappears. Thus, control theory provides a dynamic perspective to understanding human behavior. A feedback loop may be either positive or negative. A "negative" loop is one where a system (e.g., individual) attempts to decrease the discrepancy between the current and desired state and a ”positive” feedback loop would attempt to increase the discrepancy between the current and desired state. According to control theory, it is the sensing of a discrepancy and the subsequent response to eliminate or reduce it that regulate human motivation and performance (Powers, 1973). Feedback provides crucial information on the current state. While sensing any type of discrepancy is equally undesirable in mechanical systems, regardless of the direction (positive or negative), this is often not necessarily true in human systems (Carver & Scheier, 1981). Carver and Scheier (1981) suggest that positive and negative feedback results in different consequences, and that often in human systems dissatisfaction from an "over—shot" standard may not occur as it typically would when a standard is "under-shot". In the present study, the focus is on the perceived health of individuals and their help-seeking behaviors when they ”under-shoot" or are below their "health" standard. It is assumed that if an individual is healthier than one’s health standard, the "overshooting" discret behaviors to reduce th when the health standar To understand he introduce the idea of a I be strings of TOTE uni (1982) and Powers (19 connected hierarchicall superordinate system se lower-level standards In When applied to h' of control systems when than simple cybernetic complex. For examp capabilities (Carver & Sr contend that an individu nor do all individuals h l n Carver and Scheier' Scheier, 1979b; Carver 8 82 "overshooting“ discrepancy is not likely to produce the same negative affect and behaviors to reduce the discrepancy and achieve the health ”standard" compared to when the health standard is "undershot" (i.e., the individual is not as healthy as desired). To understand how feedback loops regulate behavior, it is also necessary to introduce the idea of a hierarchical system. Miller et a1. (1960) suggested that there may be strings of TOTE units functioning within each operation’s phase. Carver & Scheier (1982) and Powers (1973) also discussed the possibility that control systems may be connected hierarchically through a system of superordinate and subordinate goals. The superordinate system sets standards or referent values for the subordinate systems, and lower-level standards must be attained prior to the attainment of higher-level standards. When applied to human systems, Powers (1973) proposed a hierarchy of nine levels of control systems where each level in the hierarchy controls different behaviors. Thus, when simple cybernetic systems are applied to human systems, the system becomes more complex. For example, individuals are limited in their information processing capabilities (Carver & Scheier, 1981; March & Simon, 1958). Carver and Scheier (1981) contend that an individual does not have the ability to monitor all possible control loops, nor do all individuals have similar abilities in carrying out the self-regulation process. In Carver and Scheier’s model of self-regulation (Carver, 1979; Carver, Blaney & Scheier, 1979b; Carver & Scheier, 1981), it is proposed that the loop engaged in depends on one’s focus of attention. Carver and Scheier (1981) suggest that there are two directions in which an individual’s attention can be focused, and that the direction influences attitudes and behaviors. Attention can be directed inward, or toward the self, in which case the individual engages in self-focus or self-attention. Attention can also be directed outward, or toward the environment. Self-focus has imf that the negative feedb blotivation to alleviate negative reaction regar salientamongself-focus self-focused individuals initiate the comparator. to facilitate this com par 1975; Carver & Scheier discrepancy is discove: behaviors to counter th At this point, one discrePolicy-reducing pr to reduce the discrepant Scheier (1982) suggest tl current and desired stat occur automatically. 1 indivi ’ dual s expectancy 83 Self~focus has implications for control theory because Carver and Scheier suggest that the negative feedback loop operates when specific attentional requirements exist. Motivation to alleviate a discrepancy requires an awareness of the standard and a negative reaction regarding the discrepancy-—both of which are suggested to be more salient among self-focused individuals (Carver & Scheier, 1982; Hollenbeck, 1989). High self-focused individuals have been found to have more salient internal standards, to initiate the comparator, or matching-tostandard, process, and to seek out information to facilitate this comparison more often than low self-focused individuals (Carver, 1974, 1975; Carver & Scheier, 1982; Gibbons, 1978; Scheier, Fenigstein & Buss, 1974). If a discrepancy is discovered, self-focused individuals are also more likely to initiate behaviors to counter the discrepancy and attain the standard. At this point, one additional dimension must be included in the goal-feedback- discrepancy-reducing process. This is an individual’s expectancy regarding his/her ability to reduce the discrepancy and future discrepancies through some behavior. Carver and Scheier (1982) suggest that an individual will attempt to reduce a discrepancy when the current and desired states are dissimilar, and that a discrepancy-reducing response will occur automatically. However, this process may be interrupted as a result of an individual’s expectancy regarding one’s ability to reduce the discrepancy. According to Carver and Scheier (1981), a self-focused individual’s expectancy of being able to match the standard impacts whether the individual will actually attempt to reduce the discrepancy or will withdraw from any such attempt (either physically/behaviorally or mentally/cognitively). Thus, in control theory, there are two distinct functions which occur: an expectancy-assessment process and a discrepancy-reduction process. The expectancy-assessment process involves synthesizing information from several sources (e.g.,physicalconstrain Once this process is cor it the expectancy to In expectancy is negative Figure 6 (Carver, process and the behavr' theory. Research has between standards an combined, researchers expectancies lead to i: expectancies lead to ear Carver, 1974; Carver, 1 &Fine, 1972; Scheier, F Will may influence 0 control, social influence the standard (Carver & The valence one h i . nflnencrng one’s discrel has a positive valence ) desired. 84 (e.g., physical constraints, social constraints, resources available, importance of standard). Once this process is completed, the theory suggests that either a positive affect will occur if the expectancy to meet the standard is positive or a negative affect will occur if the expectancy is negative (Carver & Scheier, 1982). Figure 6 (Carver, 1979) presents a flow of activities in the expectancy-assessment process and the behavioral responses that are expected to occur according to control theory. Research has provided evidence that self-focus increases the congruence between standards and behavior and that, when self-focus and expectancies are combined, researchers have found that for those high in self-focus, favorable expectancies lead to increased effort to reduce the discrepancy while unfavorable expectancies lead to early withdrawal (Archer, Hormuth & Berg, 1979; Brockner, 1979; Carver, 1974; Carver, 1975; Carver, Blaney & Scheier, 1979a, 1979b; Duval, Wicklund & Fine, 1972; Scheier, Fenigstein & Buss, 1974; Steenbarger & Aderman, 1979). Factors which may influence one’s expectancies include: prior success and failure, locus of control, social influence, and causal attributions made regarding one’s failure to match the standard (Carver & Scheier, 1981). The valence one has regarding the standard has also been recognized as a factor influencing one’s discrepancy-red ucing behavior (Carver & Scheier, 1981). If a standard has a positive valence, then the standard is assumed to be a goal or objective that is desired. According to control theory, if a discrepancy exists between the current state and the standard, then a behavioral output will likely be triggered to reduce the discrepancy. If a standard has a negative valence, then the standard is assumed to not be desired (thus, a discrepancy from the standard is acceptable). Another model of control theory (see Figure 7) presented by Campion and Lord E l NUS" HELDS do our IMIUNOI Fill iElWlOl “M “at!!! in”: “it! OISQm Figure I 85 runs 1 z -& 3 1 rates 130: scue 1‘ [S ‘ - YIELOS cut vtcaAL cremation ll‘I’LIQUON ' no 333m F0! EEMVIOI FOR BEHAVIOR l SALIENT ' * v t . 5 , smug: rwunu ' YIELOS cut YIELD: no cu: rmnmnwu Iflnmflflfi : run BEHAVIOR ran atnnvroa } V I «wwmuL : flnwmo , HAS mu man -9...“ 3! rates urtcoamtcou sch-rocus urrn anumutnummsuum s ‘ ‘a as «mama» areatrv: vnrzscz: POSITIVE vnttncz: um momma mill armpm ennunmrmn) “mam-awn “won I ’4//F<\‘\49 (fru- mts point. . . an mummu firmwmmm' ‘fiifigfiw parallel Wu ' Mm ”NW.“ at? tummy to the rlglu) mmowmn mmommn undue anamn n glékh £2601 Q3050 .Eoom ‘ .0... :00 a0 @3004 Cuswol Arousal .2361 cozmnzS aazom a 2232.500 .96.. 300 >oco30oxm czaoI oozooofitnvcfim . s 2502303.; 5.3: 95.6 .225 32:00 do 2504 £30: >50ao=¢ 3:ch the complete model . There are two types Figure 9. The firstt side of the model are side. The diagram : behavioral sciences, where variable A pre bya third variable C. the flow of activitie statement about the diagram to another. In the model in using typical statistica comparator/discrepar comparator process. there are two causal r occurs. Two variables sensor data perceived as a result of the co discrepancy exists bets it actually a flow model t proc: o the causal ri Since the flow model 4 aslied of Figure 9 W 101 the complete model developed for the present study, the model will be tested in sections. There are two types of models that are typically diagrammed like the model shown in Figure 9. The first type consists of causal models where the variables on the left—hand side of the model are hypothesized to influence the variables identified on the right—hand side. The diagram from left to right represents a causal pattern. In the social and behavioral sciences, we are capable of dealing with these types of causal relationships where variable A predicts variable B, or where A and B may be moderated or mediated by a third variable C. The second type of model is similar to a flowchart which diagrams the flow of activities and variables within some process but attempts to make no statement about the nature of the relationships as we progress from one side of the diagram to another. In the model in Figure 9, it is not possible to test the complete model all at once using typical statistical techniques (such as path analysis or causal modeling) because the comparator/discrepancy variable is not ”caused" by the two variables which enter the comparator process. Figure 9 actually represents a combination of both model types-— there are two causal models connected by a flow model where the comparison process occurs. Two variables are input into this comparison process-the goal/standard and the sensor data perceived by the individual on the current state. A new variable is created as a result of the comparison process--the individual’s perception as to whether a discrepancy exists between the input variables. The comparator mechanism in the model is actually a flow process that represents a connection of the causal left-hand side of the model to the causal right—hand side. Flow and causal models have different purposes. Since the flow model or connection is descriptive, no hypotheses are developed on this aspect of Figure 9. While hypotheses are developed for the causal parts of the model, they will have t0 bf completely causally relationships among double lines indicate the model may not research question. ( the model might fit I The model will I the health goal and relationship between will betested along wi EAP usage (see Figr individual’s perceive variable that will be c arelationship betweer based on the motivat difference between or relationship between discrepancy as a methl in“ as eXpected it has been sug eh ' WIS, are influem 102 they will have to be tested separately because the flow throughout the model is not completely causally connected. The solid lines in Figure 9 represent predicted relationships among variables in the present research that were examined while the double lines indicate aspects of the flow model which were not directly examined. While the model may not be fully tested, it still might prove to be useful in examining the research question. Once the model is tested in parts, implications of how the parts of the model might fit together will be discussed. The model will be tested in sections as follows: (I) the causal relationships between the health goal and its predictors will be tested (see Figure 10); (2) the causal relationship between the com parator/perceived discrepancy variable and EAP utilization will be tested along with the moderator and predictor variables hypothesized to influence EAP usage (see Figure 11); and (3) the correlation will be calculated between an individual’s perceived discrepancy as measured in #2 above and the discrepancy variable that will be calculated using the goal and sensor inputs to test whether there is a relationship between the actual and perceived discrepancy. Because control theory is based on the motivating influence of the discrepancy variable as calculated using the difference between one’s desired goal and current state, it is important to examine the relationship between this calculated discrepancy and an individual’s perceived discrepancy as a method check to determine whether the model and relationships might exist as expected. The Goal/Standard It has been suggested by various researchers that health behaviors, like other behaviors, are influenced by a person’s desired goals regarding health along with a few F\ iHealth r Figure Health History Health Locus of Control 103 Social Group Health Values/ Norms l Desired Health Goal Level Figure 10: Factors Affecting Desired Health Goal Level Perce Discre Persr Pres to FlQUI 10,4 Health Locus of Control Perceived v Expectancy w EAP Discrepancy Personal Support/ Pressure/Barriers to Using EAP Utilization Work Support/ Pressure/Barriers to Using EAP Figure 11: Relationship Between Perceived Discrepancy and EAP Utilization other key variables Richards, 1988) F‘ and attitude regard engaged in are infill people with whom t Nader, and Rogers . influenced by three knowledge of behavl {3) the individual’s t Locus of Control). 5 and perceived cont perceived health stat Research suppo For an individual to assumed that the ind behavior is goal~direr desired end, or a wait research focuses on t the level of health do decide to utilize an motivated to do some t r l-e.,adiscrepancy be Health has been h ' . 11,15er1, mental, and 105 other key variables (Christiansen, 1981; DiMarco, 1985; Parcel, Nader, & Rogers, 1980; Richards, 1988). For example, DiMarco (1985) suggested that an individual’s concept and attitude regarding health and illness and the nature of health-related activities engaged in are influenced by the life the person leads, his/her values and goals, and the people with whom the individual associates. According to Richards (1988) and Parcel, Nader, and Rogers (1980), whether an individual’s behavior is conducive to health is influenced by three factors: (1) the individual’s desire to maintain good health; (2) knowledge of behaviors conducive to health and which can minimize health risks; and (3) the individual’s belief that his/her behavior can influence his/her well—being (Health Locus of Control). Similarly, Christiansen (1981) found that the importance of health and perceived control were significant predictors of health behaviors and added perceived health status as a third important predictor. Research supports the conclusion that individuals’ goals and values affect behavior. For an individual to be motivated to perform or modify his/her behavior, it is generally assumed that the individual holds some goal or standard regarding that behavior; i.e., behavior is goal-directed (Karoly, 1985; Pender, 1982). A goal is defined as "an aim, a desired end, or a valued outcome" (Pender, 1982). The goal of interest in the present research focuses on the overall health standard an individual desires to maintain—-i.e., the level of health desired. It is argued in the present research that for an individual to decide to utilize an EAP, the individual must desire good health in order to be motivated to do something about his/her health when he/she recognizes a health problem (i.e., a discrepancy between desired health goal and current health state). Health has been defined by the World Health Organization as a "state of complete physical, mental, and social well-being and is not merely the absence of disease or infirmity" (Stone, 1‘. of over 400 univer employees included that most individua individuals have bee in their life (DiMarc for individuals, such physical pain, not be in control of one’s lil than physical health one’s health may be i an EAP (e.g., financ etc.). Since human b individuals will neces example, one individt once the individual hi to take more proactiv activities such as mai smoking and drinking action of internal cc from the environment Research suppor related to health beha' 106 infirmity" (Stone, 1979, p. 7). In an examination of perceived health and wellness needs of over 400 university employees, Barker (1987) found the primary concerns of employees included their physical and mental/emotional well-being. It seems reasonable that most individuals will maintain a general goal or desire to be healthy--in fact, individuals have been found to invariably rank health as one of the most valued things in their life (DiMarco, 1985). However, being healthy may take on different meanings for individuals, such as not being under high levels of stress or pressure, being free of physical pain, not being on an emotional roller-coaster, not being overweight, or feeling in control of one’s life. In EAP research, an individual’s health goal encompasses more than physical health and includes emotional and psychological or mental health since one’s health may be influenced by a variety of problems which are typically handled by an EAP (e.g., financial, marital, legal, child-rearing, care of elderly, substance abuse, etc.). Since human beings prioritize goals and behaviors available to them, not all individuals will necessarily hold the same desired health goal level (Pender, 1982). For example, one individual may be interested in doing something about his/her health only once the individual becomes ill (e.g., visit a doctor) while another individual will desire to take more proactive measures to maintain a healthy state (undertake healthy lifestyle activities such as maintaining a proper diet and sleep habits, exercising regularly, not smoking and drinking, etc.). According to Pender (1982), specific goals desired are a function of internal cognitive processes which determine "what information is received from the environment and how it will be interpreted and structured" (p. 13-14). Research supports the use of desired health goal level as a significant predictor related to health behavior (Richards, 1988). In research by Kegeles (1969) and Seeman and Seeman (1983i important motivato has also been foun behaviors (Kaplan . participate in free i desired (Attitudes T &Sagan, 1959). H. behavior appears to of locus of control a that health goals did healthy state did not exam) in research cc In the present 5 is goal-directed, it is the more likely an in being met. The leVr research to be influer and the health value. below. W The first variable tudividual’s health his see ' ms logical that the I i 107 and Seeman (1983), the saliency of health (i.e., the desired level) was found to be an important motivator in preventive/protective health behavior. Desiring a healthy state has also been found to be an important predictor in decreasing levels of smoking behaviors (Kaplan & Cowles, 1978). Other research has found that individuals who participate in free medical examinations are those who consider health to be highly desired (Attitudes Toward Co-operation in a Health Examination Survey, 1961; Borsky & Sagan, 1959). However, the relationship between desired health goals and health behavior appears to be inconsistent. In a study by Baughman (1978) on the relationship of locus of control and health goals with health status and behavior, it was concluded that health goals did not predict one’s health status or behavior. Similarly, desiring a healthy state did not correlate with the practice of a specific health behavior (breast self- exam) in research conducted by Gramse (1982). In the present study, based on a control theory view and the belief that behavior is goal-directed, it is suggested that the higher an individual’s desired health goal level, the more likely an individual will be to seek EAP assistance when the health goal is not being met. The level of an individual’s health goal is hypothesized in the present research to be influenced by three variables: health history, Health Locus of Control, and the health value/norms of one’s social group. These variables will be discussed below. H thitr The first variable hypothesized to influence one’s desired health goal level is the individual’s health history, or past health status. This variable was selected because it seems logical that the more an individual has previously experienced problems regarding his/her health, the r of good health. Wl be physically, emoti disruptions which ir these disruptions, it hidher health. Prev goals was not found. reported that some i gods following succe research to the prese Previously experient healthy state. ThUS, Hungary desired such the have a higher h health problems He . alth Locus of Con: A second variab an individual’s Healtl as the degree to whi behavior; i.e., the degr of externally (Wallst herons, 1978) 108 his/her health, the more likely the individual will be concerned about preserving a state of good health. When an individual does not feel he/she is in good health, whether it be physically, emotionally, mentally, etc., the individual frequently experiences negative disruptions which impede his/her normal lifestyle. The more frequent one experiences these disruptions, it is assumed that the individual would be more concerned about his/her health. Previous research on the relationship between health history and health goals was not found. However, studies examining the general goal-setting process have reported that some individuals raised their goals after failure while others lowered their goals following success (Campion & Lord, 1982; Kernan & Lord, 1985). If we apply this research to the present study, it might be that the more health problems individuals have previously experienced (failures), the more the individuals would desire to obtain a healthy state. Thus, the hypothesis of interest in the present research is: Hypothesis i 1: An individual’s health history will affect the health goal level desired such that individuals who have had more health problems in the past will have a higher health goal level desired than individuals who have had fewer past health problems. Health Locus of Control A second variable that has been found to have an impact on health goal levels is an individual’s Health Locus of Control. Health Locus of Control (HLOC) is defined as the degree to which individuals believe their health is determined by their own behavior; i.e., the degree to which individuals believe their health is controlled internally or externally (Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan & Maides, 1976; Wallston, Wallston & DeVellis, 1978). The general c- Theory where an i beliefs concerning ‘ theory 0f per 50m“ stimulus-response/f' (Rotter, 1975). In forces nor controlle dealt with motivatir control" to refer to ‘ internal or external According to reinforcement) is de hon or is contingent actions. The locus 0' or reinforcements as almost exclusively se events beyond the cr two extremes describ- Control, outcomes fol “‘Ck, Chance, fate, tl Complexity of forces 2 ||' Internal" locus of co own ability, skills, of more initiative in the 109 The general concept of locus of control was included in Rotter’s Social Learning Theory where an internal-external locus of control scale was developed to measure beliefs concerning the control of personal destiny. Social Learning Theory is a molar theory of personality that attempts to integrate two diverse trends in psychology--the stimulus-response/reinforcement theories and the cognitive theories of motivation (Rotter, 1975). In the social learning approach, a person is neither driven by inner forces nor controlled entirely by his/her environment (Bandura, 1971). Rotter (1966) dealt with motivation as it related to locus of control and used the term ”locus of control" to refer to the expectancy that rewards were contingent upon or controlled by internal or external resources. According to Rotter (1966), an individual’s reaction to an event (reward or reinforcement) is determined by the degree the individual perceives the reward follows from or is contingent upon his/her own behavior or occurs independently of his/her own actions. The locus of control theory postulates that individuals perceive their outcomes or reinforcements as being controlled by forces that fall along a continuum going from almost exclusively self-control (internal) at one end to being controlled primarily by events beyond the control of the individual (external) at the other end. Typically the two extremes describe individuals as follows. If an individual has an ”external" locus of control, outcomes following personal actions are believed to be attributed by him/her to luck, chance, fate, the influence of powerful others, or as unpredictable due to the complexity of forces around him/her, rather than resulting from his/her own action. An "internal" locus of control individual perceives outcomes as being determined by his/her own ability, skills, or effort (Rotter, 1966). Research has revealed that internals exhibit more initiative in their efforts to attain goals and control their environment (DiMarco, 1985). Evidence alt awareofenvironme (b) try to improve I and value their owt (Rotter, 1966). Ind characteristics at th The locus of Cr behaviors (Balch 6 reduction, birth cc immunization and p research on locus of that the majority t precautionary health if control seek mort for their health bel compared locus of c hospital p0pulation r 110 1985). Evidence also exists that those labeled "internals" are more likely to (a) be more aware of environmental cues that provide useful information influencing future behavior, (b) try to improve their conditions, (c) emphasize skill or achievement reinforcements and value their own ability, and (d) show more resistance to others’ influence attempts (Rotter, 1966). Individuals are viewed as being predisposed to behave in line with the characteristics at the end of the continuum with which they are associated. The locus of control construct has been found to be related to a variety of health behaviors (Balch & Ross, 1975; Sonstroem & Walker, 1973), including smoking reduction, birth control utilization, weight loss, adherence to medical regimens, immunization and preventive dental care (Malen, 1982; Pender, 1982). In a review of research on locus of control and health-related behaviors, Strickland (1978) concluded that the majority of research on the relationship between locus of control and precautionary health practices provides evidence that individuals with an internal locus of control seek more information on health maintenance and take more responsibility for their health behavior than externals. For example, Seeman and Evans (1962) compared locus of control and the learning of behaviorally relevant information in a hospital population and found that a person’s sense of personal control was a factor in determining the level of interest and the degree of knowledge possessed concerning his/her illness. Internally-controlled individuals possessed more knowledge about their illness than externally-controlled individuals. Others have concluded, however, that the early studies did not produce consistent results (Wallston & Wallston, 1978). For example, in studies on smoking behavior, locus of control was found to be a relevant factor where non-smokers tended to be internals (James, Woodruff & Werner, 1965) and individuals who valued health and who were internally-oriented (Kaplan & Cowle: utilization (F isch, I Wallston, l978) are The locus-of-r accept personal res reinforcement expe internal or external 1985). Therefore, it is that the initial sc measure of reinfor Therefore, to better and Maides (1976) to measure the deg behavior (i.e., is inte because it was felt t HLC scale was modi to he a unidimensic responsible for their care professionals) Multidimensional Hr the Internal Health 1 Of Control (PHLC l W . allston, Wallston, 111 internally-oriented on health were more successful in changing their smoking behaviors (Kaplan & Cowles, 1978). However, conclusions from research on birth control utilization (Fisch, 1974; Harvey, 1976) and weight loss (Balch & Ross, 1975; Wallston & Wallston, 1978) are more ambiguous. The locus-of~control construct distributes individuals based on the degree they accept personal responsibility for what happens to them, which is influenced by past reinforcement experiences. The idea that an individual develops a belief about an internal or external locus of control is now thought to be situation—specific (DiMarco, 1985). Therefore, it was suggested that one reason for the inconsistent research findings is that the initial scale was not designed as a specific health measure but as a global measure of reinforcement expectancy across a wide range of potential situations. Therefore, to better apply this concept to the field of health, Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan, and Maides (1976) developed a unidimensional Health Locus of Control (HLC) scale to measure the degree to which people believe their health is determined by their behavior (i.e., is internal or external). Another revision of the LE scale was undertaken because it was felt that the definition of externality was too broad. Thus, in 1978, the HLC scale was modified when it was found that Health Locus of Control did not appear to be a unidimensional construct——some health-externals believed fate or chance was responsible for their health while other health-externals saw powerful others (e.g., health care professionals) responsible for their health. The new scale is known as the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control (MHLC) Scale and includes three sub—scales: the Internal Health Locus of Control (IHLC) Scale, the Powerful Others Health Locus Of Control (PHLC) Scale, and the Chance Health Locus of Control (CHLC) Scale (Wallston, Wallston, & DeVellis, 1978). Individuals are still classified as being either ”internals" (”Sing since these are b0 developers of this factor used to EXPIE more of a multitudr play a significant rt 168). The new scale (1981) found that r patients who were externals. Seemanz concern placed on practicing more pos In the present they factor in inflt individuals with an 803's; however, it \ difficult soars (Hollr level desired st Of Control sub: Who score lowe COntrol subscal 112 "internals" (using the IHLC scale) or "externals'l (using the PHLC and CHLC scales since these are both, dimensions of externality). It should be noted that while the developers of this scale found it useful, they cautioned that it should not be the only factor used to explain health behavior—-that ”only in interaction with one, or preferably more of a multitude of contributing factors, will beliefs in the locus of control of health play a significant role in the explanation of health behavior” (Wallston et al., 1978, p. 168). The new scales have been administered in health research. For example, Schultz (1981) found that using the Health Locus of Control Scale, adolescent cystic fibrosis patients who were internals were more likely to arrange and keep clinic visits than externals. Seeman and Seeman (1983) found that one’s health motivation (i.e., value or concern placed on health) and an internal locus of control resulted in individuals practicing more positive health behaviors than those with an external locus of control. In the present study, the perception of control over one’s health is suggested to be a key factor in influencing one’s desired health goal level. It has been suggested that individuals with an ”internal" locus of control are less likely to reject setting difficult goals; however, it would be unrealistic for ”externally oriented” individuals to set difficult goals (Hollenbeck & Brief, 1987). The hypothesis to be examined is: Hypothesis fi 2: An individual’s Health Locus of Control will affect the health goal level desired such that individuals who score higher on the Internal Health Locus of Control subscale will have a higher desired health goal level than individuals who score lower on the internal scale or higher on either of the External Locus of Control subscales (Powerful Others or Chance). Social GFOU Heal The third set goal level are the l Present. ltWaS MP of one’s social 8T0“ goals. A norm is de beliefs, attitudes, el and psychological . exposure to peers, z isa function of OI influences (Ajzen & person or group ind and feelings) of ar socialization proces hrnction in society tl norms, attitudes, pr ETOUp (Clausen, 196 Social agents 0 ethnic group, and o 1 u ° 968a). Socralizatir 113 W The third set of variables hypothesized to influence an individual’s desired health goal level are the health values and norms of members of one’s social group-~past and present. It was hypothesized that the health attitudes and norms possessed by members of one’s social group would influence the degree individuals desired to maintain healthy goals. A norm is defined as a " generally agreed upon pattern of appropriate behaviours, beliefs, attitudes, etc.” (Winefield & Peay, 1980, p. 234). Meanings are given to physical and psychological experiences as a result of ”cultural conditioning in the family, by exposure to peers, and through the mass media" (Mechanic, 1982). A person’s attitude is a function of one’s personal nature and attitudes as well as one’s social group influences (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein, 1966). Social influence occurs when "one person or group induces a change in the behaviour (overt behaviour or internal thoughts and feelings) of another” (Winefield & Peay, 1980, p. 225). It is an individual’s socialization process which determines how the individual learns to participate and function in society through exposure to other individuals and the transmission of values, norms, attitudes, preferred behavioral patterns and expectations, and sanctions of a group (Clausen, 1968a; Goslin, 1969; Hammitt, 1984; Loy & Ingham, 1973). Social agents or social group members include an individual’s family, social class, ethnic group, and others who contribute to the development of social roles (Clausen, 1968a). "Socialization" is a continual interactive process between an individual and the values and expectations held by the larger social group (Brim, 1968; Clausen, 1968a). This lifelong process varies by the situations and socializing agents an individual interacts with (Inkeles, 1968). For example, socializing agents vary as one ages, with the family and school groups acting as primary socializing agents for children, and peers, work and other groups bec McPherson & Ken The medical etiology and health with its interest in 6 1988). An individt sheeting attitudes a norms (Pender, 19l influence the emer, examined the antec- lo-year period and childhood. Childre as though their ow importance socializa Mothers appear to 1 since a high correlat the behavior of moth Placed on health an inlllortant influence behavior" (p. 248). 1 Concept, health g0a1< 114 other groups becoming more influential as one matures (Clausen, 1968b; Loy, McPherson & Kenyon, 1978). The medical field has recognized the importance of social factors on disease etiology and health behaviors, as evidenced by the emerging field of cultural medicine with its interest in examining the influence of social factors on health behaviors (Mayer, 1988). An individual’s social group can influence an individual’s health behavior by affecting attitudes and beliefs or by forcing an individual to conform to group behavioral norms (Pender, 1982). According to Pender (1982), ”family patterns of health care influence the emerging values and lifestyles of offsprings" (p. 73). Mechanic (1978) examined the antecedents of various health and illness responses and behaviors over a 16—year period and concluded that sick-role behavior is, in part, learned during one’s childhood. Children learn through the health behaviors they observe in others as well as through their own health experiences (Pender, 1982). Rosenstock (1975) cited the importance socialization into one’s family had on an individual’s lifelong health patterns. Mothers appear to be important role models for the health behavior of their children since a high correlation was also found by Tyroler, Johnson and Fulton (1965) between the behavior of mothers and their children. Bruhn and Cordova (1977) stated the ”value placed on health and the level of knowledge about health by parents represents an important influence on how a child will be reared with respect to health attitudes and behavior" (p. 248). It is this "parental modeling" which helps a child develop a health concept, health goals, and perceive a responsibility and control over one’s own health (Bruhn, Cordova, Williams & Fuentes, 1977). Concepts of health and illness are influenced by "parents’ attitudes and actions before children understand the significance of these attitudes and actions on their own health behavior " ( also influence an children grow old information, varylf family relationship patterns of exercis interpersonal relati (Pender, 1982). He groups and within 2 and behaviors. D differently. Of particular i of others influence individual typically degree of uniformitj l“ the present reset members of his/her individual’s health E Hypothesis £2 his/her social g individual who members of his an individual v norms and valu 115 health behavior" (DiMarco, 1985). However, other members of one’s social group can also influence an individual’s attitude toward health and health behaviors. "When children grow older . . . their attitudes and habits . . . are influenced by new health information, varying social contacts, and demands made upon them by school, jobs, and family relationships" (DiMarco, 1985). ”Conversations with others regarding their patterns of exercise, nutrition habits, rest and relaxation, management of stress, and interpersonal relationships” have been found to act as cues for one’s health behaviors (Pender, 1982). Health norms may exist both within an individual’s nonwork and work groups and within an entire organization, which all act as regulators of health attitudes and behaviors. Different groups may desire to maintain health-related behaviors differently. Of particular interest in the present study is whether the health values and norms of others influence the level of health goals desired by an individual. Because an individual typically chooses members of his/her social group, it is likely that a certain degree of uniformity of goals, attitudes, and norms will prevail among group members. In the present research, an individual’s perception of the health norms and values of members of his/her social group will be examined to assess their impact on the individual’s health goal level desired. The hypothesis of interest is as follows: Hypothesis #3: An individual’s perception of the health values and norms of his/her social group members will affect the health goal level desired such that an individual who perceives high general health goals and positive health norms of members of his/her social group will have a higher health goal level desired than an individual who perceives members of his/her social group hold lower health norms and values. W The specific individual’s desire problems as well a vary in the extent hypothesized to im of Control, and the floup. The next set Model of EAP Util The second pr asensor function it environmental stimt receives feedback frt Slmptoms. Research Sistem depends on tl ltCervone, 1983; Err 116 Summary of the Health Goal Component The specific overall goal or standard of interest in the present research is the individual’s desired health goal level-~which includes the desire to avoid illness or problems as well as rec0ver from illness or problems. It is suggested that individuals vary in the extent they desire to maintain good health. Three specific variables were hypothesized to impact on the level of one’s health goal: health history, Health Locus of Control, and the health attitudes, values, and norms held by members of one’s social group. The next section will discuss the second major component of the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization—-the Sensor Function. The Sensor/Feedback Function The second primary element in the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization is a sensor function which activates the process through which an individual perceives environmental stimuli and responds to them. In the present model, the sensor function receives feedback from the environment regarding one’s present health status and health symptoms. Research has indicated that for the effective regulation of behavior, a control system depends on the interaction of goals and feedback to affect performance (Bandura & Cervone, 1983; Erez, 1977; Locke, Shaw, Saari & Latham, 1981). Feedback, therefore, is necessary along with the goal or standard in order to initiate a discrepancy-reducing response if a discrepancy exists. It is the discrepancy between the goal and the present state that leads to behavior (Winefield & Peay, 1980). According to the model, the sensing process is a dynamic one where an individual is continually monitoring his/her health status. In the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization presented in Figure 9, initially an individual’s curret health status is th comparator mecha is perceived betw. discrepancy is pen the behavioral ou status/symptoms at repeated. While this sen 0f EAP Utilization individual’s perceii Problems scale devt r1— 117 individual’s current health status is perceived by the sensor function. The individual’s health status is then compared to the standard or desired health goal level via the comparator mechanism. This sensing-comparison process is repeated until a discrepancy is perceived between the desired health goal and perceived health state. Once a discrepancy is perceived and a behavioral response occurs (e.g., seek treatment), then the behavioral outcome (i.e., from the treatment) along with the current health status/symptoms are fed back into the sensor function and the sensing process is repeated. While this sensor component is an important element in the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization, no hypothesis will be examined on this aspect of the model. An individual’s perceived current health state will be measured using the Current Health Problems scale developed for this research (see Part VII, Appendix B). The Comparator Mechanism The comparator mechanism is the third primary component in control theory. Two variables are input into the comparator mechanism to create a third variable-the perceived discrepancy between the desired and current state. Once the sensor function is activated whereby an individual perceives his/her current health status, the comparator mechanism compares the individual’s desired health goal with his/her current health status as perceived by the sensor function. The outcome of this comparison process is a perception or decision regarding whether a discrepancy exists between the desired goal and current state. According to control theory, if an individual perceives no discrepancy, then a discrepancy—reducing response need not necessarily be undertaken. Instead, the individual continues to monitor and compare his/her current health symptoms against individual’s desire decision-making I respond to the di! (modify the desirt process). It should as an individual I symptoms by com As previously using a causal mod variables which are sensor/perceived he the comparator/disr in Figure 9 is one oi connecting point t connection or com process. In the casr are the health stand. the variable coming In control the subtracting the sens hey are similar~~i.e. hudei, an examinatir between the disctell 118 symptoms against the standard. However, once a discrepancy is detected between an individual’s desired health state or goal and current health status, it is predicted that a decision—making process is activated which influences whether the individual will respond to the discrepancy either behaviorally (seek EAP assistance) or cognitively (modify the desired goal or the interpretation of symptoms through the comparator process). It should be noted that the comparator mechanism is continually re-activated as an individual monitors his/her current health status and interprets the current symptoms by comparing them to his/her desired health goal. As previously discussed, it is not possible to test this aspect of the model directly using a causal model because the comparator process is a flow—type model and the two variables which are input into the comparator/discrepancy process (the goal and the sensor/perceived health status) are not predictor or causal variables of the outcome of the comparator/discrepancy process (i.e., the discrepancy variable). Rather, the model in Figure 9 is one of a flow-chart type of model connecting two causal models and at the connecting point the model is discontinuous. Two variables are input into the connection or comparator process (an operator) and a third variable flows out of that process. 'In the case of the model in Figure 9, the variables going into the flow model are the health standard and the sensor (health status) data (shown via double lines) and the variable coming out is a perceived discrepancy variable. In control theory, the perceived discrepancy variable is typically created by subtracting the sensing function’s output from the desired goal to determine whether they are similar-~i.e., whether a discrepancy exists. Therefore, to study this aspect of the model, an examination was conducted on whether there was a correlational relationship between the discrepancy variable computed using measures of the desired goal and current health st: individual’s percey as a method check be working as expr aframework for u if there was not as been suggested her: because it may not health state whicl relationship to be t usefulness of this fr the Control Theory The behavior/t when an individual current state. In tl‘. variable, focuses on compared hlS/hel‘ df This asuect of Presented in Figure tel ‘ . C ontrol, Which c on a. 119 current health state and a variable of perceived discrepancy as measured by an individual’s perception of whether a discrepancy exists. This relationship was examined as a method check on the validity of the discrepancy variable to see if the model might be working as expected. If a strong relationship existed, then use of control theory as a framework for understanding the EAP decision process might be considered useful. If there was not a strong relationship, then control theory may not be as helpful as has been suggested here in understanding an individual’s motivation to seek EAP treatment because it may not be the actual discrepancy between an individual’s desired goal and health state which motivates a response. There is no hypothesis covering the relationship to be examined here, but a method check will be conducted to verify the usefulness of this framework. The next section will present the last main component of the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization—-the behavior/effector component. The Behavior/Effector Component The behavior/effector component is the discrepancy-reducing response undertaken when an individual perceives there is a discrepancy or error between the desired and current state. In the present study, the behavior of interest, which is the dependent variable, focuses on whether an employee utilizes EAP services after an individual has compared his/her desired state with his/her current health state. This aspect of the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization to be tested is presented in Figure 11 where it is hypothesized that two variables will moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization: (1) Health Locus of Control, which consists of three subscales or variables, and (2) expectancy of goal attainment. Two additional variables, personal support/pressure/barriers and work support/pressure/bart regardless of whether below. In general, it is depends on whether Control and a high 0 Based on the model i of Control (HLOC) Sl likely to behaviorally an individual is low ( HLOC subscales (Por then the individual perceived. A cogniti health goal or altering Present study is on ' assistance. The varia discussed below. He alth Locus of Cont Health Locus of affecting the level of a e - O xamrne this variable bet ween a perceived ( assistance at an EAP 120 support/pressu're/barriers, are hypothesized to have a direct effect on EAP utilization, regardless of whether a discrepancy is perceived. These relationships will be discussed below. In general, it is suggested that whether an individual responds to a discrepancy depends on whether an individual possesses an internal or external Health Locus of Control and a high or low expectancy that the EAP will help lead to goal attainment. Based on the model in Figure 9, if an individual is high on the Internal Health Locus of Control (HLOC) subscale or perceives a high expectancy, then the individual is more likely to behaviorally respond to a discrepancy and utilize EAP services. However, if an individual is low on the Internal HLOC subscale or high on either of the External HLOC subscales (Powerful Others or Chance) or perceives a low level of expectancy, then the individual is more likely to cognitively respond when a discrepancy is perceived. A cognitive response might be in the form of modifying the individual’s health goal or altering the interpretation of health symptoms/status. The interest in the present study is on whether the individual responds behaviorally by seeking EAP assistance. The variables hypothesized to influence the behavioral response will be discussed below. Health Locus of Control Health Locus of Control has previously been discussed as a variable potentially affecting the level of an individual’s desired health goal. The present research will also examine this variable as to whether it has a moderating influence on the relationship between a perceived discrepancy in a person’s health and the decision to seek health assistance at an EAP. As previously mentioned, individuals tend to hold primarily an internal or external individuals believe th Kaplan & Maides, 19 external locus of co determined by luck, « due to the complexitj onone’s own action. as being determined l is more assertive in sl Health Locus of the health behavior de personal behavior car health. Lau (1982) ut its three subscales-hi control over health w unless an individual be and future health, Ii rtSponsibility for his/l. Ongoing researc] associated with h motivation, reduv utilization of hea use of alcoht Health Locus of t f Wild that nonsmoker ani ‘ nternal orientation 121 internal or external view of power. Health Locus of Control is the degree to which individuals believe their health is determined by their own behavior (Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan & Maides, 1976; Wallston, Wallston & DeVellis, 1978). If an individual has an external locus of control, outcomes following one’s actions are perceived as being determined by luck, chance, fate, the influence of powerful others, or as unpredictable due to the complexity of forces around him/her, rather than being entirely contingent on one’s own action. An individual with an internal locus of control perceives outcomes as being determined by his/her own ability, skills, or effort (Rotter, 1966) and generally is more assertive in shaping his/her environment (Pender, 1982). Health Locus of Control was chosen as potentially having a moderating impact on the health behavior decision process because it involves an individual ’s belief that his/her personal behavior can make a critical difference in the individual’s present and future health. Lau ( 1982) utilized the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control scale (with its three subscales--Internal, Powerful Others, and Chance) and concluded that self- control over health was a good predictor of self-care behavior (p. 328). It seems that unless an individual believes his/her behavior can have a direct impact on his/her current and future health, little motivation will exist to seek health assistance and take responsibility for his/her health. According to Rosenstock (1975): Ongoing research suggests that external control, alienation or powerlessness are associated with higher rates of morbidity, lower rates of compliance, lower health motivation, reduced tendency to seek behaviorally relevant information, reduced utilization of health care services, and reduced ability to control weight, smoking and use of alcohol and other drugs (p. 135). Health Locus of Control was examined in studies on smoking behavior, and it was found that nonsmokers and those who quit smoking tended to be more likely to have an internal orientation (Best, 1975; Coan, 1973; James, Woodruff, & Werner, 1972; Platt, 1969; William been examine For example, externally con success in a r oriented. Ht correlation be in other reported more l972), ln add in tescarch 0n lime COmparet ‘0 PaillClpants Price (1982) wt ml" I10npartic; There is e The lOIlOWing s examined in tin relatitmshi are high Or belWeen th are l0w on 122 1969; Williams, 1972 as cited in Pender, 1982, p. 63). Health Locus of Control has also been examined in relation to weight and weight loss; however, the results are equivocal. For example, O’Bryan (1972) found that overweight individuals tend to be more externally controlled than those of normal weight, and Balch and Ross (1975) concluded success in a weight loss program was found to occur more among those internally oriented. However, Bellack, Rozensky and Schwartz (1974) found no significant correlation between weight loss and locus of control. In other health-related research, individuals who are internally controlled have reported more frequent use of seat belts than those externally controlled (Williams, 1972). In addition, the importance of an individual’s HLOC has also been supported in research on the use of birth control where internals tended to practice birth control more compared with externals (McDonald, 1970). Finally, the MHLC was administered to participants and nonparticipants in a work site fitness program by O’Connell and Price (1982) who found that participants in the program were more internally oriented than nonparticipants. There is evidence that locus of control is relevant to predicting health behaviors. The following set of hypotheses, which are outlined in Figures 12a to 12¢, are to be examined in the present research: Hypothesis #4a: Internal Health Locus of Control (HLOC) will moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. Individuals who are high on the Internal HLOC subscale will respond to a perceived discrepancy between their desired and current health by utilizing an EAP more than those who are low on the Internal HLOC subscale. EAP Utilizat Fighe 12a: Hypothesis #4a Yes High IHLC EAP Utilization N0 Low IHLC L H Perceived Discrepancy Figure 123: Hypothesized Relationship of Internal Health Locus of Control (IHLC) Utilizar l‘lElll‘e 12b: 124 Hypothesis #4b Yes Low PHLC EAP Utilization No High IHLC L H Perceived Discrepancy ___¥ Figure 12b: Hypothesized Relationship of Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (PHLC) EAP Utilizatiol Figure 120: H: 125 Hypothesis #40 Yes Low CHLC EAP Utilization No High CHLC L H Perceived Discrepancy .g Figure 12c: Hypothesized Relationship of Chance Health Locus of Control (CHLC) M the re” who are discrel)a than tho} 35mg relations} are 10‘” 0 between t are high C The seeom health discrepan which refers to 1 goal; i.e., the bf Expectancies ar e initiated, the amt (Bandura, 1977b; Expectancy behavior is inflUi Individual will act (expectancy). Cot eillectaneies in d 126 flypgjhesfiifiib: Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (HLOC) will moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. Individuals who are low on the Powerful Others HLOC subscale will respond to a perceived discrepancy between their desired and current health by utilizing an EAP more than those who are high on the Powerful Others HLOC subscale. Hypothesis #4c: Chance Health Locus of Control (HLOC) will moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. individuals who are low on the Chance HLOC subscale will respond to a perceived discrepancy between their desired and current health by utilizing an EAP more than those who are high on the Chance HLOC subscale. Expectancy The second variable hypothesized to moderate the relationship between a perceived health discrepancy and the discrepancy—reducing response is the individual’s expectancy, which refers to the individual’s perception that a given action will achieve the desired goal; i.e., the belief that a specific health action will prevent or ameliorate illness. Expectancies are considered to be important in determining whether behavior will be initiated, the amount of effort exerted, and whether effort will be sustained over time (Bandura, 1977b; 1982). Expectancy ties into Rotter’s (1954) social learning theory, which postulates that behavior is influenced by the expected probability of an outcome occurring. An individual will act if he/she believes the behavior will lead to the goal/reinforcements (expectancy). Control theory models have also emphasized the importance of outcome €Xpectancies in determining whether (a) behavioral or cognitive changes occur (e.g., ——7 127 Taylor et al., 1984) and (b) whether the individual withdraws or persists (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981). As previously discussed, self—regulation is also a key concept in control theory. The concept of expectancy is relevant to the self-regulation of health process since an individual’s expectancy regarding a particular health behavior has been found to have an important influence on behavior and on the direction of actions. For example, one’s expectancy of being able to eliminate a discrepancy (e.g., high blood pressure) in order to meet the goal (e.g., normal blood pressure range) can have an influence on the type of outcome that occurs (i.e., whether an individual takes medication, modifies diet, etc. depends on whether the person feels the behavior will reduce his/her blood pressure). In the present research, expectancy refers to the employee’s perception of the likelihood that utilizing an EAP will help lead to the achievement of the individual’s health goal and the reduction or elimination of the perceived discrepancy between the desired and current state. According to the model developed in the present research, if an individual’s expectancy regarding the attainment of his/her goal is high, meaning that he/she believes the seeking of assistance at an EAP will improve one’s current and future health state and reduce the perceived discrepancy, then it is more likely the individual will seek EAP assistance. However, if an individual’s expectancy regarding the EAP’s ability to achieve the health goal is low or poor, then the individual will decide to not make use of EAP services. Expectancy that an EAP will help red uce/resolve health problems can be influenced by an individual’s expectations regarding the benefits to seeking EAP assistance. Thus, the more benefits perceived by an individual to using EAP services, the higher one’s perceived expectancy might be regarding the EAP’s ability to help. Prior research bet hm mhe hpr phi emp] Wat [he 5| Piiso‘ experi influe] reiatio E regirth expirie mtllre ( i 128 examining health behaviors has identified a number of benefits or motivators that have been found to influence not only an individual’s belief that seeking assistance will help but also influence the actual seeking of assistance. Some of these benefits or motivators for health and help-seeking behaviors include: (1) a desire for social approval (Gochman, 1971; Haefner & Kirscht, 1970; Pender, 1982); (2) a desire to avoid disease, improve health, or receive approval from significant others (Antonovsky & Kats, 1970); (3) the ability of the program to provide strategies to reduce stress, reduce depression, improve one’s self—concept, improve overall health, improve one’s work performance/career (Straussner, 1986), help employees recover from alcoholism, save employees’ jobs, improve morale, save money (Straussner, 1986), promote psychological well~being, and decrease anxiety (Sidney & Shephard, 1976). In addition to the above motivators or benefits that might influence one’s belief in the success of an EAP (i.e., expectancy), another factor hypothesized to influence a person’s expectancy is an employee’s prior experience with an EAP and whether that experience was positive or negative. Prior experience has been found to be a factor influencing one’s future behavior (Bandura, 1982; Carver & Scheier, 1981). For example, Ajzen and Fishbein (1980) suggested the strength of the attitude—behavior relationship may be moderated by the direct experience one has with the object. Expectancies have been found to increase after success and fall after failure regarding the specific behavior (Feather, 1966, 1967; Feather & Saville, 1967). Prior experience regarding usage of an EAP has also been found to influence an individual’s future expectancy on the EAP’s ability to assistemployees. Pardue (1987) found that V6 hig bel ex]: i 129 of 200 clients surveyed, 98% stated they would recommend the EAP to others, 71% were very satisfied with the service provided, and 85% reported they experienced improvement in their problem situation. Ford and McLaughlin (1981) found that the highest group willing to use EAP services was the group of former EAP users. In the present research, expectancy is hypothesized to moderate the health-seeking behaviors of an individual. Therefore, the hypothesis to be examined regarding the expectancy variable, which is presented in Figure 13, is: Hypothesis # 5: Expectancy regarding the ability of the EAP to resolve or improve an individual’s perceived health problems will moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. Once a discrepancy is perceived between an individual’s desired and current health, an individual with a high expectancy regarding the EAP will seek assistance from the EAP more than an individual with a low or poor expectancy. The next variables hypothesized to influence whether an individual seeks assistance t an EAP are the perceived social support or social pressure along with perceived arriers from both work and non-work sources that encourage/discourage the individual i make use of EAP services. House (1981) has defined social support as a "flow of strumental aid, information, and/or appraisal (information relevant to self-evaluation) ‘ tween people” (p.26). Social support involves interpersonal transactions that include ‘I e or more of the following elements: affect, affirmation, and aid (Kahn & Antonucci, , 30). Others have identified different types of social support in terms of the functions ved by each type: emotional, informational, instrumental, and belongingness (Cohen 130 Hypothesis #5 Yes High Exp'y EAP Utilization No Low Exp'y L H Perceived Discrepancy Figure 13: Hypothesized Relationship of Expectancy (Exp'y) 131 Hoberman, 1983; Spacapan, 1988). People within an individual’s social environment (e.g., family, social group, peer Jup, school, coworkers, supervisors, etc.) may provide strong support in goal :ainment and problem-solving in a variety of social situations (Loy, McPherson & anyon, 1978; Wynne, 1986). Haskell and Blair (1980) emphasize that the attitudes and actions of those with whom a person interacts determine whether an individual will rticipate and adhere to some activity. Similarly, Fishbein (1966) predicts a person will gage in a specific behavior if the behavior is evaluated positively and if the individual rceives that others within his/her social group think it should be performed. This bjective norm may exert pressure to perform or not perform a behavior that may be lependent of a person’s attitude toward the behavior (Hammitt, 1984). Therefore, e’s personal perception of the social pressure to perform or not to perform a behavior 1 key influence on behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). For example, Anderson and firm ( 1973) examined the health behaviors of students enrolled in a university health 0 and found that the more positive students perceived the attitude of others was ard the student health center, the more positive was the students’ own view toward health services, which in turn influenced their utilization of services. Of interest in )resent research is the encouragement/discouragement received by other individuals rding the seeking of assistance and the utilization of EAP services. Also of interest are the perceived barriers to using an EAP. While individuals may re an EAP can help reduce/resolve their problem(s), they still may not seek ince because of perceived barriers or costs. These barriers might also be classified ro categories-~those perceived to exist that are not related to the work environment ose that are related. ———f 132 In the present research, it is hypothesized that social support/pressure from both individual’s personal life as well as work life along with perceived barriers related to e’s personal and work environments will directly influence the decision to seek help. 5 suggested that these variables will directly influence an individual’s behavior rather in moderate the relationship between a perceived discrepancy and EAP usage because individual may seek EAP assistance due to pressures from sources in one’s work or n-work environment even when the individual does not perceive there is something mg with his/her health (a discrepancy). However, when combined with barriers, ial support/pressure may have a different influence on EAP utilization. These nbined variables will be examined in the present research. In this research, support/pressure from the work environment was separated from port/pressure received from other individuals in one’s personal life because it has 0 suggested that these may be separate constructs. Mayer (1988) noted that there has 1 sparse evaluation of management support as a significant variable in health arch and that there appears to be no empirical evidence that management support 1 significant effect on health promotion efforts at the work site. Mayer notes that :social system variables, such as norms and social support, have been examined as air influence on health behavior and suggests that management support is often porated into the global construct of "social support,” which generally refers to all as and forms of support behavior provided to an individual, rather than being Jed as a separate concept. Mayer and others suggest that social support from friends, and significant others one socializes with is not the same construct as :ment-based support within an organization (Broadhead et al., 1983; Bruhn & 1987; Mayer, 1988). Therefore, the present research will examine social ———’f 133 support/pressure regarding the seeking of assistance at an EAP that is received from individuals both outside and within the workplace. In addition, as previously mentioned, barriers to using an EAP can also be classified into those related to one’s personal or work environments. Sources of support/pressure that might be received from individuals within one’s personal life along with personal barriers will be discussed first and then possible sources of support/pressure and barriers from the work environment will be presented. Personal Sgurces of Support/Pressure and Barriers There are several sources of social support within an individual’s personal life which might influence the person’s behavior. These include support from parents and relatives, friends, and others with whom an individual socializes with outside the workplace. Social support has been examined in health research and found to influence articipation in health activities (Hammitt, 1984; Merriman, 1984; Snyder & Spreitzer, 973), adherence, and changes in health behaviors (Terborg, 1988 as cited in Mayer, 988). However, while social support has been examined in health research, such as its ect on participation and adherence to treatment, (Broadhead et al., 1983; Leavy, 83), Terborg (1988, as cited in Meyer, 1988) suggests the results from this research are uivocal. Blackwell (1979) believes that the family plays an important role in reinforcing pr0priate health behaviors and eliminating inappropriate health behaviors. Caplan 76) found that individuals reporting high levels of social support tend to perceive ir hypertension treatment as more beneficial than costly. Individuals who discussed tal problems with their family and friends have also been found to be more likely to 134 Leek preventive dental visits compared with those reporting no such discussions Antonovsky & Kats (1970). Haskell and Blair (1980) found that pressure from one’s ocial group influenced the success of motivating individuals to maintain a more active festyle. Warren (1982) indicates that health-seeking behaviors often require informed :ferral agents to encourage the seeking of assistance. He estimates that 90% of those ho self-refer for help do so after talking with a natural helper. In EAP research, tampion (1988) demonstrated that external sources of support influence whether ents follow through on EAP referrals. In another study by Pardue (1987) on 200 users an internal EAP, clients stated their awareness of the EAP’s existence came through npany publications (30%), company training (24%), fellow employees (17%), and a Jervisor (11%). While most clients in the Pardue (1987) study stated they were self— arred (79%)--primaril y because they saw their own job performance deteriorating-~the raining used the EAP as a result of suggestions by the supervisor, fellow employees, ’amily members. In addition to support, encouragement, and pressure from sources in one’s personal hat might influence an individual’s decision to seek assistance at an EAP, there may be a number of barriers or obstacles that exist that can influence an individual’s ion to not seek EAP assistance. A number of EAP experts have identified several ers that might deter EAP usage. The present research has classified some of these ers as being from “personal” sources, which means they are barriers perceived by dividual to exist outside of his/her workplace. Some of these barriers include: 1) inconvenience, unavailability, or difficulty in attending the EAP (Ultsch, 1983); 2) competing responsibilities (Langlie, 1977); 135 (3) a stigma associated with many problems which an EAP addresses (e.g., lcoholism, drug dependency, and mental and behavioral problems) where an individual afraid friends/family will perceive the person as being weak or deviant (Roth, 1981; rich, 1980). Such stereotyping has been associated with mental health issues unnally, 1961), where an employee may be perceived as pathological, uncooperative, curable, untreatable, or has having some inherent character defect because of such oblems (Mierkiewicz-Alleva, 1986); (4) financial problems, such as poor insurance benefits or limited coverage for some .atments (Dickman & Emener, 1982; Straussner, 1986); (5) perceived barriers regarding the EAP itself: limited EAP staff availability, lack 24-hour coverage, lack of attention on prevention (Straussner, 1986), failure to wide services to meet all the person’s needs, lack of follow-up (Dickman & Emener, l2; Straussner, 1986), lack of confidence in. the EAP staff (Hung, 1988), and luck of appropriate relationship with community mental health, drug, and alcoholism cies (Thoresen, 1978); (6) lack of knowledge on how to contact the EAP; (7) availability of other resources (Marino, 1985). It is hypothesized that the combination of both support/pressure and barriers from sonal" sources will have a direct influence on an individual’s decision to utilize EAP ces. This new "combined" variable called "personal support/pressure/barriers" will amined in the present study. In the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization Oped in the present research, it is predicted that this variable may influence an idual’s seeking of EAP assistance regardless of whether the individual has actually ived a discrepancy in his/her health between the desired and current health state. 136 or example, while an individual may not think he/she has a health problem, such as hen an alcoholic denies such a problem, the person may still go to an EAP because tis/her spouse encourages/pressures him/her to go. However, if the person also perceives )0 many barriers to seeking assistance, then despite this spousal pressure, the person aay still not seek assistance. The hypothesis to be examined is: Hypothesis fi 6: An individual’s perception of the combination of support/pressure and barriers from "personal" sources to seeking EAP assistance will have an effect on the individual’s utilization of EAP services. Individuals who perceive high levels of support or pressure to seek EAP assistance and few barriers will tend to have contacted the EAP for assistance more than individuals who report low levels of support or pressure and many barriers to seeking assistance. irk-related Sources of Support/Pressure and Barriers The second form of support and barriers which might influence an individual’s ision to seek EAP assistance occurs at the workplace. Glasgow and Terborg (1988) Syme (1986) indicate that organizational factors are likely to influence occupational th interventions and outcomes. Individuals at the work place are a captive audience workers, managers/supervisors, members of the labor union, and the organization Four different types of support at the work site will be discussed: from coworkers, diate manager/supervisor, unions, and the organization itself. Following this ssion, work—related barriers to EAP utilization will be discussed. While work group s (Coburn & POpe, 1974; Green, 1970; Kirscht, 1983) and management support 137 have frequently been cited as two key organizational factors influencing behavior (Beer, 1980; Everly & Feldman, 1984; Felix et al., 1985; French & Bell, 1984; Mayer, 1988), empirical evidence regarding their influence on EAPs is minimal (Mayer, 1988). The first potential source of work site support/pressure is an individual’s coworkers >r work group. Gottlieb ( 1982) views coworkers as natural helpers at the workplace who :an help an individual change a situation by providing assistance and information about vailable services. Coworkers can also serve as influential mediators between the 'oubled employee and professional assistance and can either block or facilitate the path > assistance. Work groups can provide rewards or sanctions in order to influence the ealth behaviors of members. Social group attitudes and norms are informal regulators of behavior that have been und to be an important factor in understanding organizational behavior (Albrecht & oldman, 1985; Blau & Scott, 1962; Burawoy, 1979; Goldman, 1983; Gouldner, 1954; itz & Kahn, 1966). As discussed above, the health norms and values of members of individual’s social group were hypothesized to influence the individual’s desired lth goal. In this section, it is suggested that other individuals’ norms may also uence health behaviors of employees. For example, work group norms have been nd to influence both health benefit use and health-related absences from work yer, 1988). Mayer (1988) found that employees’ perceptions of pro-health work p norms were related to fewer absences for illness and injury and lower health s/costs compared to work groups with adverse health norms. The findings, ever, were inconclusive regarding the overall impact since Mayer (1988) concluded work norms had no influence on health risk behaviors (smoking, etc.). Allen and n (1986) examined negative health norms and found such norms can have a negative 138 impact on health behaviors and the reduction of health risks. In the present study, work group attitudes toward seeking assistance will be examined as one form of support or pressure generated to influence another’s health— related behavior. Employees who are in a work group that does not support seeking assistance and encourage good health behaviors and the use of the EAP may be discouraged from using EAP services for fear of job loss, alienation or ridicule from others. A second potential source of work site influence on an individual’s behavior is the immediate manager or supervisor. Management support has been recognized as an important factor influencing work behaviors related to work site health promotion programs (e.g., Everly & Feldman, 1984; Felix et al., 1985; Orlandi, 1986). It seems logical that managers would also play a key role in influencing employees in seeking EAP assistance. Past EAP research has identified management support as a critical factor impacting on EAP effectiveness (Dickman & Emener, 1982; Greenwood, 1983; hain & Groeneveld, 1980) and also on the number of supervisory referrals made to EAPs (Myers & Myers, 1985; Roman, 1984). A third potential source of work site support or pressure may come from the labor mica/labor stewards. While supervisors and coworkers may have an impact on one’s lecision to seek EAP assistance, for the utilization of EAP services to occur, the full articipation and support of any organized labor group/union within the organization required (Gordon, 1973; Trice & Roman, 1972). According to Trice and Roman: An established fact of industrial relations is that management programs involving employee welfare must have the full consent and cooperation of the labor union pggther employee organizations if they are to be effective and durable (1972, p. )p stewards as well as coworkers and supervisors are often able to identify fellow ———i 139 employees in trouble and to maintain a closer working relationship-thus, an employee may be more open to advice from the union or peers compared to a supervisor. Therefore, labor is in a good position to encourage an employee’s seeking of treatment, and labor’s opposition would result in a reluctance on the part of employees to self— 'efer. A last source of support or pressure at the workplace to be discussed comes from he organization itself. This form of support encompasses the extent to which an mployee perceives that the organization encourages or discourages the employee to taintain his/her health and seek EAP treatment when needed. As previously tentioned, human beings learn to participate in society through a process called icialization, defined as "an interactional process whereby a person acquires a social entity, learns appropriate role behavior, and in general conforms to expectations held ' members of the social systems to which he belongs or aspires to belong" (Loy & gham, 1973, p. 258). Organizational socialization occurs at the work site to regulate lployees’ behavior through a process whereby employees learn the organization’s eptable norms (rules) of behavior. Mitchell and Hurley (1981) indicated that "an ortant area for future research is how various social settings influence the elopment of helping networks and supportive transactions” (p. 295). Some settings promote or deter guidance and support. An organization can develop social norms just as an employee’s work group does, these also play a big part in human behavior in organizational settings. nizational health norms differ from the work group health norms in that the nizational health norms are a much broader concept and consist of an employee’s ption regarding the organization’s general attitude toward health and toward the i 140 EAP in particular. Work group norms are focused on a narrower field of perception in that only the group of coworkers the employee regularly interacts and works with are considered part of his/her work group. Glasgow and Terborg (1988) and Syme (1986) state that it is very likely that organizational factors affect work site health programs and their outcomes. Research has suggested that organizational support is needed for the success of EAPs to occur (Dickman & Emener, 1982; Greenwood, 1983; Shain & Groeneveld, 1980). Organizational sponsorship and support of a program helps legitimize a project, provides rewards, and helps to control worker behaviors, just as the withdrawal of this support or sponsorship from a project causes the new work behaviors to decline (Crockett, 1977; Frank & Hackman, 1975; Mayer, 1988; Miller, 1975). More employees are likely to use EAP services when managers are given the support and authority they need to act in their referral role, and when the program is given high visibility throughout the organization with adequate training provided on implementation, use, and location (Googins & Kurtz, 1981). Wrich (1978) claims that regardless of the work setting (e.g., 'ndustrial, government, university), EAPs are established to help people with problems nd the key ingredient is whether an environment is created that will encourage oluntary referrals. Such an environment stresses trusting relationships, a nonpunitive rogram policy, confidentiality, absence of labels to users, provides a choice of action r those seeking assistance (Wrich, 1978), a climate of acceptance of trust, Open mmunication, and a control system that optimizes individual freedom (Crookston, 75). Mayer (1988) examined health promotion activities at the work site and proposed at some organizations are more likely to develop strong norms to support health 141 ctivities while others may develop equally strong norms to discourage them. However, e found that supportive organizational health norms had no relationship with mployees’ health behaviors. The only empirical research found in the literature review hich specifically examined EAP usage and organizational support was conducted by ung (1988) who measured employees’ perceptions of their organization’s climate in neral. Organizational climate may be defined as ”a set of measurable properties of the ork environment, perceived directly or indirectly by the people who live and work in is environment and assumed to influence their motivation and behavior" (Litwin & ringer, 1968, p. 2). Hung did not examine employee perceptions regarding a specific ganizational "health " climate or norm; rather, a "general" climate measure was utilized (1 climate was then dichotomized as being either "warm" or "cool”. What is :eresting about this research is that Hung (1988) found that EAP use was not nificantly related to employees’ climate perceptions, although many employees nmented on the lack of their company’s concern for its employees as a barrier to .P usage. The research by Mayer (1988) and Hung (1988) contradicts the traditional assertion organizational support is essential in influencing employee behaviors. However, e Hung (1988) concluded the perception of a warm or cool organizational climate no influence on actual EAP usage, Hung did conclude that organizational climate eptions had an influence on responses by both EAP users and non-users regarding future willingness to use an EAP--with those perceiving a warmer climate being willing to use the EAP in the future if needed. Organizational climate also need perceptions of whether using the EAP would hurt one’s career where oyees perceiving a warmer organizational climate strongly disagreed that using the 142 AP would hurt their careers (Hung, 1988). While support and pressure from work-related sources might influence an dividual’s decision to seek assistance at an EAP, there may also be a number of lfl'leS or obstacles that can be classified as ”work—related” that can deter an individual )m seeking EAP assistance. Some of these barriers have been identified by EAP perts and include: (1) a stigma associated with seeking assistance where an employee may be labeled weak or deviant by his/her peers if they find out the employee sought help or the ,ployee fears that using the EAP would be held against them-—e.g., future promotions, . (Nunnally, 1961; Roth, 1981; Wrich, 1980); (2) perceived lack of confidentiality--which remains a primary concern of both ployees and EAP providers (Dickman & Emener, 1982; Lovenheim, 1979; Marino, 5; Pardue, 1987); (3) lack of support by all organizational levels—veg, supervisors, unions, t0p agement (Beyer & Trice, 1978); (4) inaccessibility to entire workforce or to dependents (Straussner, 1986); (5) failure to provide services to meet all the needs of employees (Dickman & ner, 1982; Straussner, 1986); (6) lack of union support or labor—steward training (Dickman & Emener, 1982); (7) inadequate communication to all employees regarding the EAP--the existence, tives, functions, and services (Greenwood, 1983; Shain & Groeneveld, 1980); 8) belief that problems should be resolved outside the workplace by the employees elves (Marino, 1985). f particular interest in the present research is the influence that work-related ————i 143 sources might have on an employee’s utilization of an EAP. It is postulated that employees’ perceptions regarding the concern for their health from managers, coworkers, labor unions, and the organization and the support/pressure received to seek assistance is important in influencing employee behavior. In addition, the perceived barriers within the workplace are also predicted to influence an employee’s decision to seek assistance. It is hypothesized that the combination of both support/pressure and barriers from ”work” sources will have a direct influence on an individual’s decision to utilize EAP services. This new ”combined” variable called “work support/pressure/barriers” will be examined in the present study. It is hypothesized that this variable may influence an individual’s seeking of EAP assistance regardless of whether the individual has actually perceived a discrepancy in his/her health between the desired and current health state. The hypothesis to be examined is: Hypothesis #7: An individual’s perception of the combination of support/pressure and barriers from "work" sources to seeking EAP assistance will have an effect on the individual’s utilization of EAP services. Individuals who perceive high levels of support or pressure at work to seek EAP assistance and few barriers will tend to have contacted the EAP for assistance more compared with individuals who report low levels of support or pressure and many barriers to seeking assistance. Human behavior is very complex and control theory is one theoretical framework at has been applied to help us better understand behavior. Control theory has been 144 used in research in a variety of disciplines, such as engineering, applied mathematics, economics, organizational behavior, clinical and health psychology, etc. This chapter began with an overview of control theory which emphasizes goals, feedback (current state), and goal-feedback discrepancies as key influences on an individual’s behavior. Various models based on a control theory perspective were presented along with a escription of the basic control theory components (Campion & Lord, 1982; Carver & Scheier, 1982; Klein, 1989; Miller, Galanter & Pribram, 1960). The present research is focused on a specific health behavior (EAP usage) and :xamples of how control theory has been applied to the field of health were discussed. ‘he physical and social environments in which an individual lives have profound effects n the individual’s concepts and attitudes toward health and illness. Health behavior : influenced by several interrelated variables including cultural, social, psychological 1d organizational determinants. Identifying and understanding the underlying causes ’ health behavior should help us develop and implement more effective health ograms. A Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization deveIOped for the present research 3 then discussed (see Figure 9) along with the hypotheses to be tested. The basic ponents of the model were presented as they relate to the present research on EAP ization: the goal/standard, sensor function, comparator mechanism, and the avior/effector. The overall goal or standard of interest in the present study is an ividual’s health goal and it was hypothesized that three variables would influence an ividual’s desired health goal level: health history, Health Locus of Control, and social p health values/norms. Two variables were also hypothesized to have a moderator ence on EAP utilization: Health Locus of Control (with three subscales) and 145 expectancy of goal attainment. Finally, personal and work sources of support/pressure and barriers were hypothesized to directly influence seeking EAP assistance. The next chapter will outline the method to be used to test the hypotheses presented in this chapter. IIIIIIll.-------—————____fi CHAPTER 4 METHODOLOGY This chapter describes the data and methods used to examine factors influencing employees’ decision to utilize or not utilize EAP services. The variables to be studied have been conceptualized based on a control theory framework (refer to Figure 9). Power Analysis A power analysis was conducted to determine the sample size required in order to acquire an adequate level of power to detect significant effects. The hypotheses presented in the preceding chapter were tested primarily with regression analysis. The statistic of interest is the standardized regression coefficient. Assuming that the standardized regression coefficient for the moderator explains at least a small effect (R2=.10, Cohen & Cohen, 1983), it was determined that 137 individuals would provide a power of .80 at an alpha level of .05 based on seven independent variables (determined using the section of the model to be tested that includes the greatest number of independent variables). Barker (1987) notes that the usual rate of return by university employees for mail surveys is approximately 30 percent. Therefore, to account for a 30% return rate, questionnaires had to be mailed to at least 457 employees. In the present study, participants were categorized into two groupsr-those who had used an EAP and those who had not. In addition, analyses of employees in the group that had not used an EAP were to be conducted only on those who might be classified as ”needing" some form of EAP service (if individuals have no health problems, there ‘1 46 77 147 would be no reason for them to seek any assistance and, therefore, they should not be included in the analyses comparing individuals who use/do not use an EAP despite having similar problems). It has been estimated that up to 30% of an employer’s workforce has serious personal problems (Cahill, 1983; Egdahl & Walsh, 19890; Weiner et al., 1973; Wrich, 1980b). To ensure this 30% was included in the analyses, it was determined that the non-user group would be divided in half based on the level of current problems being experienced. Utilizing 50% of the group in the analyses (those with more problems) was viewed as a conservative approach (i.e., if only 30% potentially require EAP services and significant results are obtained by including an additional 20% that may not require assistance, then more confidence might be placed in the findings). To ensure an adequate number of surveys would be returned by employees who had used and not used an EAP, the following number of questionnaires were mailed to employees: 300 to EAP users and 600 to non-EAP users (to account for the fact that only half of those returned from non-EAP users would be utilized in the analyses). Research Site Michigan State University (MSU) was the data collection site selected. This research site was chosen for several reasons. First, the characteristics of the university’s labor force made it ideal to examine participation/non-participation in an EAP, since there was a large proportion of full-time employees, a large number of males and females, and a variety of occupational levels that included clerical, maintenance, and supervisory employees. Second, a positive response for conducting the research was received from university personnel, particularly from the EAP staff, who indicated the present area of research had not been studied extensively in the past. Third, the ——'— 148 accessibility of the EAP to the majority of employees minimized the constraints of time, distance, and cost. Fourth, the use of a single research site provided a means of controlling for the potentially confounding effects of organizational and logistical differences extraneous to the research. Finally, there has been a great deal of concern about the plight of the troubled employee in the post-secondary educational setting. The primary resources in higher education are human resources. Researchers have suggested there is an urgent need for colleges and universities to implement the EAP concept and provide services for the recognition, treatment, and rehabilitation of employees whose behavior affects their work performance (Trice, 1980; Trice & Roman, 1972; Von Wiegand, 1974). Post-secondary educational institutions rely on the quality and well—being of the staff, who are subject to the same personal and work problems that employees working in other business and industrial settings face. While industrial organizations have been dealingwith the mental and emotional health of employees for almost a century, the programs are relatively new in higher education. Due to the labor intensity within a university setting and the need to maintain healthy human resources, programs have been implemented in institutions of higher education and today there are approximately 300 university-based EAPs. Wrich (1978) suggests that the impact of programs established by universities is much greater on the community at large than all the money the federal government could Spend promoting such programs on its own. University programs not only help the employees directly involved in their usage but the community as a whole due to the inter-relatedness and interdependence of us all. Michigan State University established an internal (on-site) employee assistance program as a benefit available at no cost to employees and their immediate family 149 members to assist them with personal problems or difficulties that could negatively affect job performance in order to help "employees resolve their difficulties and return to a more satisfactory work and personal life“ (Employee Assistance Program Services, 1990). Like most EAPs, the MSU program had its beginnings in a substance abuse program developed in the 1970’s which included such services as confidential professional consultation, referral, and follow-up care. By the late 1970’s, the program was expanded to encompass a broad range of personal problems, such as personal, medical, psychological, work-related, substance-abuse, and financial and legal problems (though individuals may be referred to other resources depending on the type and severity of the problem). In 1985 the EAP became a division of the Department of Human Relations and the program was expanded to include prevention and educational services along with its existing assessment, consultation, and referral services. The university’s internal EAP is a broad-brush model located in a building on the university’s campus which is easily accessible to employees. Initial consultations/interviews were scheduled with an EAP counselor at a convenient time for the employee and information revealed by an employee through consultation with the EAP staff does not become a part of the individual’s employment record. Sample The final sample consisted of full-time employees of Michigan State University who had the availability of the EAP as part of their benefits package and who had equal accessibility to EAP services (i.e., employees were located on campus). In order to obtain an adequate sample of employees who had utilized the EAP and to be able to Compare these employees with similar employees who had not utilized the EAP, EAP ———f 150 utilization was examined by union groups at the university and employees who were members of the unions with the highest EAP utilization rates were selected. It is argued that if union/employee groups were selected whose utilization was extremely low, then there would not be enough variance in the dependent variable (EAP usage) to adequately examine differences between EAP users and non—users. Following are the union/employee groups selected along with their respective EAP utilization rate for the fiscal year from July 1, 1989 to June 30, 1990: the Administrative Professional Association (APA) and Administrative—Professional Supervisors’ Association (APSA) unions with a combined utilization rate of 10.3%; the Clerical—Technical (CT) union with a 12% utilization rate; the American Federation of State, County, and Municipal Employees (AFSCME) Local 1585 with a 4.6% utilization rate; and the AFSCME Local 999 with a 14.5% utilization rate. A list of university employees who were classified as being members of the selected union/employee groups was obtained from the university. The employees were then classified as being EAP users or non—EAP users based on data provided by staff from the university’s EAP. A total of 900 employees were then randomly selected from within these two groups as follows: 100 EAP users and 200 non-EAP users from the APA/APSA group, 100 EAP users and 200 non-users from the CT group, and 100 EAP users and 200 non-users from a combination of both AF SCME (999 and 1585) locals. Questionnaires were returned from 426 employees for an overall return rate of 47%, or 59% for EAP users and 42% for non-EAP users. Of these 426 questionnaires, reSponses from 406employees were examined (12 surveys were returned late and 8 were incomplete). Of the 406 surveys examined, there were 168 EAP users and 238 non-EAP users. The non-user group was then divided approximately in half based on the median 151 level of current problems (where 131 were classified as employees with the lowest level of current health problems and 107 were classified as having the highest level of current health problems). The final sample included in the analyses consisted of 168 EAP users and the 107 non-EAP users with the highest level of current health problems, for a total of 275 employees. General characteristics of the final sample are provided in Tables 2 and 3. Frequencies were run on all demographic variables, and Table 4 displays the total number and percentage of individuals possessing each characteristic by EAP usage/non- usage. Crosstabulation analyses were also conducted to examine the respondents’ characteristics in more detail. This procedure provides information about the relationships between variables-i.e., about their independence. A Pearson chi-square statistic is obtained to test the hypothesis that the variables are independent. 1f the statistic is small enough (e.g., < .10) then the hypothesis that the two variables are independent is rejected. In the final sample, there was a significant disparity between the job position held and gender where females tended to hold primarily clerical positions while males were more distributed among four of the other positions-- maintenance, skilled, professional, and managerial (p < .01). No significant differences were found in the sample when examining race and gender, marital status and gender, job position and race, job position and marital status, and marital status and race. Crosstabulation analyses were also calculated to compare EAP usage/non-usage on the various demographic factors. In this sample, there was no significant difference between EAP users and non-users on race, education, job position, age, and tenure with the organization. 152 Table 2: Descriptive Statistics of Sample Variable Code N* Percent Mean Median ,§Q_ Gender 1.74 2.00 .44 Male 1 71 25.8 Female 2 204 74.2 Race 1.18 1.00 .68 White 1 250 90.9 Black 2 9 3.3 Hispanic 3 4 1.5 American Indian 4 5 1.8 Asian -5 4 1.5 Other 6 2 .7 Missing Value 1 .4 Education 4.04 4.00 1.27 Some High School 1 3 1.1 High School 2 45 16.4 Trade School 3 26 9.5 Some College 4 101 36.7 Undergraduate 5 63 22.9 Graduate 6 36 13.1 Missing Value 1 .4 l l Job Position 3.51 3.00 1.51 i Maintenance 1 31 11.3 Skilled 2 23 8.4 Clerical 3 114 41.5 Technical 4 19 6.9 Professional 5 47 17.1 Manager 6 37 13.5 Other 7 3 1.1 Missing Value 1 .4 Marital Status 3-74 3-00 1.24 Single w/o children 1 72 26.2 Single w/children 2 40 14.5 Married w/o chldren 3 52 18.9 Married w/children 4 111 40.4 * Total N=275 153 Table 2 (cont'd) Descriptive Statistics of Sample Variable Code N* Percent Mean Median §Q_ Family Income 4.51 4.00 1.65 Less than 310.000 1 1 .4 ~310,000 — 319.999 2 22 8.0 ‘520,000 — $29,999 3 74 26.9 330,000 - $39,999 4 55 20.0 $40,000 - $49,999 5 36 13.1 350.000 - $59,999 6 32 11.6 $60,000 Or More 7 53 19.3 Missing Value 2 .7 Age 4.49 5.00 1.96 Under 25 1 9 3.3 26 — 30 2 45 16.4 31 - 35 3 42 15.3 36 _ 4O 4 41 l .9 41 - 45 5 52 18.9 46 — 50 6 43 15.6 51 - 55 7 22 8.0 56 ~ 60 8 15 5.5 Over 60 9 6 2.2 Tenure At Organization 4.13 5 00 1 11 f Less than 1 year 1 4 1.5 l 1 to < 3 years 2 30 10.9 3 to < 5 years 3 38 13.8 5 to < 3 years 4 55 20.0 8 Or More Years 5 148 53.8 * Total N=275 ti . illlf. illuiuiin- five-(ulhcnhUCI-Q: sic Himmzx QHQEWW aHO QOHUWHUMNUW ”KrHHAHHaHOWMVQ MAW “Hank 154 l o=~m> mnwmmwz mm.a mm.a mm.m mm.m mm.m it- wN.H ms.H e masseuse Hm.” 4H.e No.8 ms.m Hm.e it- oH.H Hs.H m muaaeaHMHmeea am.m mH.v mH.e we.“ me.“ it- 8H.H ms.H a meHHoo meow em.e mm.e mH.a mm.m mo.m ll- HH.H em.H m Hoozom means sv.s sm.e mo.a we.~ mm.~ -lt HH.H ow.H w Hooaom :mHm se.¢ oo.w se.m mm.~ mm.H tit so.m be.H H Hoonom :mHm meow Gawuwosvm ous> mcwwwfiz guano om.m mm.m om.e oo.m co.e cm.e it- oo.m m :an< ov.e em.v as.» ov.~ oe.m cv.m it- cm.H v eaHeeH eaoHcma< mm.e m~.e mm.e om.m ms.~ om.m it- ms.H m oHeaamHm mm.e mm.v mm.e em.~ HH.4 we.v it- oo.m N xoaHm vH.¢ sv.e nm.a es.~ Hm.” vo.e it- ms.H H oHHaz 006m eo.e «4.4 mm.v se.m we.” no.4 mm.H it- w mHe-om sm.e me.e me.e mm.m vH.m ao.e so.H it- H mHa: hwvcwc mh=GOS rMMfl OEOOCH mzuwum 00H. 056m mow”— xow 0300 munwwhwb HaHHcez Hocoo oflznwumosoa :o umwwm chH>oam meanneswoamn mo mcwozv emaiam mo nowumwuwum mbwuawaommn “m mHnae 155 l g o:~m> mchmHz «v.v mw.v tit we.” mm.v b>.v NH.H ve.~ b one: no ooo.ocn m©.m mw.v tit Hm.m mm.m mH.v mo.H mm.H m www.mm» l coo.om« mm.w mm.v tit am.m Ho.m wa.m mH.H m>.H m www.mve t coo.ov» mm.v mv.v t-l mv.m um.m m~.v wH.H uc.~ v www.0mm t ooo.omw vH.v mm.v tit mc.m vw.m em.m mm.H Hm.H m amm.ama r coc.om« mo.m cm.m tit «w.H ow.m we.» mm.H Hm.H N ama.a~9 i ccc.cn» oo.m oo.m tit oo.H oo.m oc.v oo.fi co.m a occ.oH9 can» wwwq osoocu hafiswm mH.w Nm.v mm.m tit mo.” wH.v mH.H o>.H v cmuUHH:0\3 vowemwz mw.v mm.m Hm.m til we.” $0.5 NH.H H>.H m nonufiso O\3 cmwuuwz mm.m cm.” w~.m int o~.m mm.m mH.H om.H N :ouUHH:0\3 omuch oH.v ee.v en.” -t- we.” «H.s sm.H ms.H H emHeHHao ox: oneHm mauwum kuHch osz> mchmH: Hague mv.v mw.v vw.w no.m tit H~.m 0H.H on.” o pounce: mm.» mv.v mm.m Hw.m tit >w.v mH.H o>.H m Hmeofimmomonm m>.m mm.» HH.v mm.~ at: 0H.v mm.H vb.H v Hwoqccoos wo.v ww.e mo.v me.m it: we.» «H.H om.H m Haowaoao am.v oa.v co.m «5.x at: m~.m «N.H «N.H m coHHme ®~.v Hw.v m>.m Hm.m tit mv.m mm.H mm.H H mocaaoucwa: :oHuHmom new cusses .mum. mucosa mauoum .mmm. 056m coax -mmm. ovoo oHauch> tit HmuHecz Amoco ofiznmamosoo :0 606cm nouw>oam moflzmeMoseo mo memos» muniwm we mowumwueum o>wuq«hommo He.u:oov e oHnee AUsUCOOM Mu H‘nflunh 156 o~.m cm.N om.m vH.H me.H m whom» one: no w tit HH.v mo.v ©v.m mm.m mH.v Hm.H w>.H v memo» w v 0» m tit ww.m no.v we.m mm.m vH.v mH.H vw.H m mace» n v o» m tit bv.m om.v bo.m «v.m cm.v mo.H us.H m mecca n v OH H it- om.m cm.e oc.m ms.v om.m oo.w co.m H can» H ameH mmmH :oHuwNchwno u< masses >©.v tit oo.v om.m mm.~ pH.m so.H so.H m aw Ho>o om.v tit mo.v oo.~ ew.~ uv.m mH.H mu.H m cm t on Hw.v tit mm.v vc.m mw.m mm.m mm.H p>.H 5 an t Hm vv.v tit mo.m Hm.m mo.v mv.v 5H.H o>.H m cm i we bm.v it: Hm.v om.~ mo.m mm.w mm.H uw.H m we r He vm.v tit mm.v mm.m mm.m ww.m mH.H wu.H v ov r em mm.m it: am.v mo.m Hw.m Ha.m co.H om.H m mm i Hm om.m tit ac.v mu.~ vv.m mm.v om.H me.H m cm t on uc.m iii oo.v mm.m HH.m HH.v oo.H w>.H H mm coca: owe Mg Mmfi 2.505 macaw dig awrzlfim coax xmw enco 3 iii HwHHemz Hmuoo onqmnonoa no woman wouH>oum moHaqumoaoa mo memos. oHstm no moHumHuwum w>HuqHHomen Hu.u=oov m mHaae 157 Table 4: Frequencies of Demographic Variables by EAP Usage/Non-usage Total Non—EAP Usage EAP Usage Variable N N x N % Gender Male 71 37 .52 34 .48 Female 204 70 .34 134 .66 Race White 250 96 .38 154 .62 Black 9 6 .67 3 .33 Hispanic 4 O O 4 1.00 American Indian 5 2 .40 3 .60 Asian - 4 2 .50 2 .50 Other 3 1 .33 2 .67 Education Some High School 3 3 1.00 O 0 High School 45 15 .33 3O .67 Trade School 26 13 .50 13 .50 Some College 101 37 .37 64 .63 Undergraduate 63 25 .40 38 .60 Graduate 36 13 .36 ' 23 .64 Missing Value 1 1 1.00 Job Position Maintenance 31 18 .58 13 .42 Skilled 23 11 .48 12 .52 Clerical 114 38 .33 76 .67 Technical 19 8 .42 11 .58 Professional 47 20 .43 27 .57 Manager 37 11 .30 26 .70 Missing Value 4 1 25 3 75 Marital Status Single w/o children 72 28 .39 44 .61 Single w/children 4O 8 .20 32 -80 Married w/o children 52 23 .44 29 .56 Married w/children 111 48 .43 63 .57 158 Table 4 (cont'd) Frequencies of Demographic Variables by EAP Usage/Non—usage Total Non~EAP Usage EAP Usage Variable N N s N % Family Income Less than $10,000 1 1 1.00 O 0 $10,000 - $19,999 22 6 .27 16 .73 $20,000 - $29,999 74 27 .36 47 .64 $30,000 - $39,999 55 18 .33 37 .67 $40,000 ~ $49,999 36 19 .53 17 .47 $50,000 - $59,999 ~ 32 20 .63 12 .37 $60,000 Or More 53 16 .30 37 .70 Missing 2 2 1.00 Age Under 25 9 4 .44 5 .56 26 — 30 45 14 .31 31 .69 31 - 35 42 18 .43 24 .57 36 - 4O 41 15 .37 26 .63 41 ~ 45 52 2O .38 32 .62 46 ~ 50 43 15 .35 28 .65 51 - 55 22 9 .41 13 .59 56 — 6O 15 7 .47 8 .53 Over 60 6 5 .83 1 .17 Tenure At Organization Less than 1 year 4 2 .50 2 .50 1 to < 3 years 30 13 .43 17 .57 3 to < 5 years 38 12 .32 26 .68 5 to < 8 years 55 20 .36 35 .64 8 Or More Years 148 60 .41 88 .59 159 Human Rights To insure protection of employees’ rights, approval for this study was obtained from the Human Subjects Committee of MSU. A letter identifying the purpose of the study was included with the questionnaire to employees for their approval. Employees were informed they would derive no benefits nor incur any risks from the study and complete confidentiality was guaranteed. Study Design/Procedures The research explored a self-motivated health behavior-EAP utilization--and was conducted in a natural setting using a cross-sectional, nonexperimental research design since no variable is manipulated and the individuals could not be randomly assigned to the EAP usage or non—usage group. Regarding the value of nonexperimental research designs, Abdellah and Levine (1979) stated: An important value of nonexperimental research is the broader scope that such studies can have, since it is less costly to use large samples of study subjects than in experiments. Therefore, more independent variables can be studied, with perhaps a greater depth of analysis possible than in an experimental approach to the same problem. Moreover, the artificiality of the experimental situation is eliminated, thereby providing findings that can have more relevant application to the real world (pp. 237-238). The research involved the collection of data by means of a survey mailed to the employee sample described earlier. It was mailed during the Spring 1992 academic term. The questionnaire packets contained: (1) a cover letter explaining the project and use of the data; (2) an informed consent form; (3) an explanation of the voluntary nature of participation and a statement informing participants that they could decline to answer any or all questions; (4) the questionnaire; (5) a letter (A or B) on the cover of the survey used to identify the respondent as a member of the EAP user group (A) or the *7 160 non—user group (B); (6) assurance of confidentiality and an explanation that results would not be included in any personnel record or provided to the university for any reason; (7) an offer to pay respondents $5.00 upon return of the questionnaire; and (8) a stamped, addressed return envelope for direct mailing of responses to the researcher via U.S. Mail. Pilot Tests Three pilot tests were conducted. The first pilot study was conducted to determine the potential for obtaining enough employees who would volunteer to participate in a research project examining their health and health—related behaviors. Seventy employees were randomly selected from the university’s staff phone book and mailed a letter in August 1990 describing the proposed research and asking whether they would be willing/unwilling to participate in such research if they were randomly selected. Responses were obtained from 40% (n==28) of the original letters mailed. Of those returned, 75% (n =21) of the employees stated they would be willing to participate, 18% (n =5) stated they would be unwilling to participate, and 7% (n =2) were returned with responses that the employee was no longer employed at the university. A second pilot study was conducted to determine whether employees who had actually utilized the EAP would be willing to participate in the research to see how willing they would be to admit to EAP usage and to respond to questions about their health. A letter describing the proposed research and requesting their response to whether they would be willing to participate was given to all employees who came to the EAP offices for assistance during a one-week period in September 1990. Because this week happened to be the first week of the Fall academic term at the university, which ———i 161 traditionally is a low EAP—usage week, and because there was only one EAP counselor working during that week due to recent turnover, only six letters were distributed to clients and returned to the researcher. However, all six employees responded positively, thus 100% stated they were willing to participate in the research if randomly selected in the future. Based on these first two pilot tests, approval was given by the dissertation committee to continue the research project. A third pilot study was conducted to test the format, factor structure, and reliability of the survey instrument prior to administering it to the final sample. There were 145 college students from management courses who volunteered for this pilot study in exchange for research credit provided by their instructor toward their course grade. The questionnaire tested in this pilot study included items for all measures except for those included in the support/pressure/barriers scales (discussed below). Participants attended two sessions approximately two weeks apart during which they completed the same survey in order to examine the stability (Cronbach alpha or test-retest reliability) of the measures. Most of the items and scales included in the final questionnaire were those found to have a reliability of .70 or higher from this pilot test. These items and scales will be discussed below in the "Measures” Section. A fourth pilot test was conducted to examine the items developed for the perceived support/barriers/pressures scales (personal and work-related items). It was felt that actual employees would be the appropriate sample to test these items on. MSU employees were randomly selected from among the three employee groupings utilized in this study. Three-hundred questionnaires were mailed along with a cover letter to the following employees: 100 to employees who were members of the APA and APSA unions (50 employees in each union), 100 to employees who were members of the CT —7— 162 union, and 100 to employees who were members of the 999 and 1585 AFSCME unions (50 employees in each). The overall return rate was 39% (n=118 with 115 fully completed surveys returned). Items included in the final survey are reported in the following section regarding the Support/Pressure/Barriers scales. Measures This section describes the measures used to operationalize the variables in the study. The independent variables are: health history, Health Locus of Control (3 subscales), social group health values/norms, general health goal, self-focus, current health status, perceived discrepancy between the individual’s health goal and health status, expectancy of goal attainment, and personal and work-related support/pressure/barriers to using the EAP. The dependent variable is a measure of whether the employee has used or not used EAP services. All measures are included in Appendix B. Each variable will be discussed based on results from the pilot studies conducted above and from the analyses conducted on the surveys returned from the final sample (i.e., 168 EAP users and 107 non-EAP users). Descriptive statistics of the variables discussed below are provided in Table 5. Health History. A series of questions regarding an individual’s past health were develOped for purposes of the present research. The following areas of health were focused on: general physical health, head, cardiovascular/respiratory, eyes/ears/nose/throat/mouth, and miscellaneous. Of the 72 questions included in the pilot study, the most reliable items (test-retest reliabilities ranging from .70 to .94) most representative of the above health areas were included in the final survey (see Part III of the survey in Appendix B). Test-retest reliability was calculated and used to 163 HmuHammH an amp» HoHHQ scum uchHsonov haHHHanHcm «appearance kuHsmme zvsuw chHm soap vmustonov auHHHneHHmm caaH< sowacono Hey HHmonm HoH Haoo suHuma emHHmmn Hav as. mw.e oo.m4 we.H4 sm-4m eH new mauaZ\mm=Ha> sHHaoe HeHoom H_Hmm .mm :chmz can: owzam mnswmoz :H 44 mHseHnw> immuuH mo Hoausz Hove: one :H onanaw>aonwx no moHumHuwum c>HuQHh0mon u m oHnwh iiiiiiiiiiiiiiiHHuIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII----------i-- i 164 determine the items to include because it was not expected that the items included in health history measure were similar enough to utilize Cronbach alpha reliability-—i.e., just because an employee may have high blood pressure does not necessarily indicate the employee also has cancer, diabetes, liver disease, allergies, etc. However, in the final study, Cronbach alpha reliability was calculated based on the responses from actual employees (this was low at .62). Employees responded either yes/no to 14 health problems presented in the survey and also responded to an open-ended question where they were asked to identify the number of additional health problems they had experienced that were not included in the survey. An overall health history score was obtained for each individual by adding up the number of "yes” responses along with the number of additional problems identified in question 15. Scores ranged from 0 to 15. Health Locus of Control. Rotter (1966) devised one of the first instruments to measure locus of control. This I—E scale consisted of 29 forced-choice items which has received extensive validation (Herring, 1987; Rotter, 1975). To better apply this concept to the field of health, Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan, and Maides (1976) developed a unidimensional Health Locus of Control (HLC) scale to measure the degree to which people believe their health is determined by their behavior and is under their control (i.e., is internal or external). The [-1 LC scale was composed of 11 items. Individuals with high HLC scores were "health-externals" who perceived factors such as luck, fate, chance, or powerful others determined their health. Low scorers were labeled "health— internals” for those who believed one became healthy or sick as a result of one’s own behavior. The HLC was based on Rotter’s (1966) I-E Locus of Control Scale but used a Likert-type scale response format (rather than a forced-choice format) and was found —’7 165 to correlate .33 with the I-E Scale (Wallston et al., 1976). Wallston, Wallston and DeVellis (1978) employed the HLC scale on other samples and found that the original alpha reliability of .72 decreased to somewhere in the range of .40 to .54. The dimensionality of the scale was then re-examined and, based on the work of Levenson (1974), the scale was modified when it was found that some health- externals believed fate or chance was responsible for their health while other health- externals saw powerful others (e.g., health care professionals) were responsible for their health (Levenson, 1974). Therefore, three new scales were constructed: internal, powerful others, and chance. Wallston, Wallston, and DeVellis (1978) further developed and tested this more specific tool, which they referred to as the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control (MHLC), with the idea that: ”by assessing more than one dimension of Health Locus of Control, the probability of increasing understanding and prediction of health behaviors could be increased“ (p. 167). The three sub-scales were referred to as the Internal Health Locus of Control (IHLC) Scale, the Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (PHLC) Scale, and the Chance Health Locus of Control (CHLC) Scale, where l the last two are dimensions of externality. All three subscales were used in this study. Internal Health Loeus of Control (IHLC) is the perception that the reinforcement for participation in the EAP is contingent upon the individual’s own behavior. Mu; Others Health Loeus of Control (PHLC) is the perception that reinforcement for participation in the EAP is under the control of powerful others. gimme of Cpntrol (CHLC) is the perception that reinforcement for participation in the EAP is a result of chance. Form A of the MH LC scale was selected for this study which contained a total of 18 statements (six from each of the three dimensions). Internal consistency values, or 166 alphas, which were reported by Wallston, Wallston, and DeVellis (1978) for the IHLC, PHLC, and CHLC scales were .77, .67, and .75 respectively. Wallston and Wallston (1981) reported test-retest reliabilities (over a four- to six-month period) for the three scales as .66, .71, and .73 respectively. Results of the third pilot test indicated reliability was lower than Wallston and Wallston (1981) reported on two of the scales: IHLC .78, PHLC .67, CHLC .66. The MHLC scales are intended for use with adults with at least an eighth grade reading level and no functional impairment (Malen, 1982). Each scale consists of six items which utilize a six- point Likert-type scale that ranges from "strongly agree“ (1) to "strongly disagree” (6), with no neutral midpoint so respondents were forced to make a decision: This was done to eliminate the inability of a person to make a decision as a variable in scale development. The scores on each scale can range from 6 to 36. The higher the score for each dimension (36 being the highest obtainable for any dimension), the more indicative of that particular locus of control influence. Total IHLC, CHLC, PHLC scores were calculated for all individuals by adding up scores from the six items in each scale. The MHLC scales can be found in Part I of Appendix B (Form A). Questions included in the IHLC subscale are 1, 6, 8, 12, 13, 17; questions included in the PHLC subscale are 3, 5, 7, 10, 14, 18; questions included in the CHLC subscale are 2, 4, 9, 11, 15, 16. Reliability for the three subscales based on the final sample (IHLC .64, PHLC .58, CHLC .67) was found to be lower than that calculated from the pilot test results. Other descriptive statistics on the three scales are provided in Table 4. Social Group Health Values/Norms. This scale measures employees’ perceptions of the health values and norms (past and present) of members of their social group (parents, friends, spouse, coworkers, etc.). This scale examines the following areas: 167 general health norms, physical health, mental health, nutrition and. fitness, lifestyle/habits, and current influence. Of the 34 items pilot-tested, 17 were included in the final measure (see Part V, Appendix B) based on test-retest reliabilities calculated on items in the third pilot study (reliabilities of the 17 items ranged from .70 to .75). Cronbach alpha reliability calculated on the final survey responses was .79. Responses to each question were scored so that the more positive the response, the higher the score assigned to that response. For example, if a question had five responses to choose from and the first one, or "a", was the most positive regarding health influence, then this response was assigned a score of ”5" while the least positive response, or ”e", was assigned a score of "1". Item responses were then summed to obtain a final score. The higher the score, the more positive the individual perceived the health value/norms of his/her social group. Scores ranged from 24 to 57, with a mean of 41.68. Desired Health Goal Level. An individual’s desired health goal level refers to the desired level of health aspired to. As stated in the previous chapter, it seems reasonable that most individuals will maintain a general goal or desire to be healthy. Thus, in order to measure the degree individuals vary in their desired health goal, it was felt that it would take more than just asking employees ”Do you desire good health?“ because of the demand characteristics present. Therefore, employees were asked about their desires and aspirations regarding specific aspects of their health (e.g., weight, nutrition, smoking, sleep, stress level, etc.). Of the 17 items included in the pilot test, 12 were included in the final measure (see Part VI, Appendix B) based on test-retest reliabilities (which ranged from .65 to .81). Test-retest reliability was used to examine items in this scale because it was not expected that the items would be internally consistent-~i.e., just because an individual desires to not smoke or drink alcoholic beverages does not mean 168 the individual also desires to regularly exercise or refrain from eating unhealthy snack foods. (Cronbach alpha reliability was calculated on the final survey responses, and it was .38). Individuals’ scores ranged from 25-55 (see Table 4). Responses to each question were scored so that the more positive the response, the higher the score assigned to that response. Item scores were then summed to obtain an overall Desired Health Goal Level score where the higher the person’s score, the greater the health goal level desired. Current Health Status. Health. status refers to the physical and mental well—being of employees (Levey & Loomba, 1984). Health status was operationalized with a series of questions focusing on the following areas of health: physical, mental/emotional, nutrition and fitness, lifestyle/habits, and general. Employees were asked to measure their current health on several items using a Likert—type scale ranging from ”Very Poor" (1) to ”Excellent" (5). Of the 71 items pilot-tested, 37 were included in the final questionnaire (see Part VII, Appendix B). Cronbach alpha reliability calculated on the final responses was .97. Employees’ responses to the items were summed to obtain an overall Current Health Status score. The lower the number, the more health problems perceived by the individuals. Scores ranged. from 76-185 (for EAP users and non—users in the final sample). Other descriptive data are shown in Table 4. This variable was used to determine initially which of the non—EAP using employees were to be included in the analyses. The group of non-users who responded (total n =238) were divided based on their overall Current Health Status score (refer to the ”Power Analysis" section above for an explanation). While the intent was to divide the group based on the median score (median = 153), upon examination of the data a more natural dividing point in the sample was indicated at a score of 162. Therefore, 169 all employees whose score was less than 162 were included in the final non-EAP usage group (n==107) while the remaining employees whose health was perceived as better were not included in any additional analyses (n =131). Health Goal—Health Status Discrepancy. In a control theory model, it is predicted that an individual is motivated to act based on the existence of a discrepancy between his/her goal and his/her current status regarding that goal. In the present research, the discrepancy of interest was the perceived discrepancy between a person’s desired health goal level and current health status. Unfortunately, the variables in the current EAP control theory model are not quantifiable as they often are in other control theory studies so calculating a discrepancy cannot be calculated in an objective manner. (For example, in a quantifiable measure, if one’s production goal is 20 pieces per hour and the current rate is 15, then there is a discrepancy of 5.) Two measures were developed to examine the existence of a discrepancy for use in the present research. The first measure developed was used for the ”perceived discrepancy" variable identified in Figure 11 in order to test this aspect of the model. For each of the items included in the Current HealthStatus measure, employees were asked to identify the change in their health which they desired on each of the items using a five-point scale ranging from ”No Change Desired” (1) to a “Very Large Change Desired” (5) (see Part VII, Appendix B). An employee’s discrepancy score was calculated by summing the item responses. The higher the score, the more an employee perceived a discrepancy between his/her current and desired health. Scores ranged from 27 to 188 with a mean of 74.59 (see Table 4). Cronbach alpha reliability of the final survey responses was .97. The second discrepancy measure was calculated by standardizing the scores on the Desired Health Goal Level measure and the Current Health Status measure to examine 170 whether each participant’s score on each variable fell within the same percentile (e.g., upper 25th, 51-75th, 26-50th, or lower 25th on both measures). If both scores for an individual did not fall within the same percentile, a discrepancy existed. For example, if a person’s Desired Health Goal score was within the upper 25th percentile (high health goal level), a discrepancy between the desired and current health existed if the person’s Current Health Status score did not also lie in the upper 25th percentile. Quartile cut-off scores for the Desired Health Goal Level measure were under 41, 43, 46, and over 46 and cut-off scores for the Current Health Status scale were under 138, 152, 160, and over 160. Based on the quartile scores, individuals were classified as either having a discrepancy or not. This discrepancy measure was used as a method check to examine whether the model and the relationships hypothesized existed by correlating it with the ”perceived discrepancy" measure calculated above using the responses on the ”Desired Change" scale in order to examine whether the control theory framework might be useful in guiding this type of research. Expeetaney of Goa] Attainment. Perceptions have been the basis of most research on health-protective behaviors, with particular emphasis on beliefs (Ultsch, 1983). Expectancy in the present research refers to the perception or belief an individual has that utilizing the EAP will help attain his/her desired health goaI--i.e., eliminate the perceived discrepancy between desired and current health. In the present research, expectancy was measured by asking participants whether they perceived going to an EAP would help resolve the specific health problems they were experiencing, as identified in their Current Health Status survey. Individuals responded to each item based on a scale ranging from "Very Poor/EAP Would Be of No Help” (1) to “Excellent/EAP Would Help a Lot" (5). An individual’s scores on all items 171 were summed to obtain an overall Expectancy score. Scores ranged from 1 to 172 with a mean of approximately 94 (see Table 4). Cronbach reliability based on responses from the final survey was .98. WW. Two scales were created to measure perceived support/pressure/barriers to using the EAP--a scale representing items from sources or barriers perceived in one’s personal life (parents, friends, other relatives, self) and from sources or barriers perceived in one’s work life (supervisor, coworkers, union, the organization). The questions in these scales addressed participants’ perceptions regarding others’ encouragement of, discouragement of, and involvement in EAP participation along with various pressures or barriers which may influence the use of EAP services. A high score on either scale indicates that a high level of support and few barriers were perceived from the particular source (personal or work). The Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers scale was developed based on the fourth pilot test discussed above that was conducted using 115 MSU employees (note that pilot- study participants were not included in the final sample). This pilot test was conducted in order to develOp weights in analyzing the final survey data for each source of support or pressure or barrier category. Actual employees were used in the pilot study because it was felt that their responses to perceived support and barriers would have greater external validity for determining the items best to include in the final survey to be mailed to other ”similar" employees. A number of support and barrier items were included in the pilot test. In order to determine which items may be perceived as more important in influencing EAP behavior, weights were calculated based on responses from the pilot which were then used to calculate scores for the personal support/pressure and barriers scales for employees in the final study. —”f 172 To calculate these weights, pilot study participants were asked to respond to 24 statements regarding perceived support, encouragement, discouragement and barriers from sources in their personal lives (i.e., friends, spouse/significant other, family, and others) using a six-point scale ranging from "Strongly Disagree" (1) to “Strongly Agree" (6). Ten statements were related to "personal“ sources of perceived support/pressure/encouragement. For each statement all individuals’ scores were summed (e.g. Item 1 scores for individuals 1 to 115 were summed together for a total item 1 score). Items were then grouped together based on the particular source (i.e., three questions referred to support from friends, two related to spouse, two to family and two to other sources). Item scores were then added together for each source and an average score was calculated per source. Next, the 14 items relating to "personal“ barriers were divided into eight categories and item scores were added together for each category and averages were calculated. Finally, an overall percentage was calculated for each personal source/category in order to obtain weights to assign to each for use in analyzing results from the final survey responses (i.e., a percentage was calculated by summing all averages to use as the total score and calculating the percentage of this total accounted for by each personal category). Refer to Part IV in Appendix B for the final items included in the personal and work support/pressure/barriers measures. The measure of personal sources of support/pressure calculated for the final sample consisted of six items with weights based on the above pilot results (see Part IV, Appendix B, for items 1, 4, 5, 9, 11, 12, weighted .027, .022, .034, .022, .038, .028 respectively). Individuals’ weighted scores were summed on these six items to create the Personal Support/Pressures scale where the higher the score the more support perceived. The final measure of personal barriers consisted of #1 173 seven items (2, 3, 6, 7, 8, 10, 13) weighted according to the pilot results (.036, .029, .038, .030, .033, .038, .031, respectively). Cronbach alpha reliability for the Personal Support/Pressures scale was .81. Individuals’ weighted scores were summed on these seven items to create the Personal Barriers scale where the higher the score the more barriers perceived. Finally, one score was developed to include all the "personal" related perceptions regarding support/pressure and barriers to using EAPs in order to examine the overall influence of people and things (barriers) from personal sources. This overall Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers variable was calculated by subtracting the total personal barriers score for each individual from his/her total personal support/pressure score. This score was then interpreted as the higher the score the more support and fewer barriers perceived from personal sources, and this score was used to examine the impact of personal influences on EAP utilization. A similar procedure was used to develop weights for the Work Support/Pressure/ Barriers measure. Participants in the fourth pilot study were asked to respond to 28 statements on perceived support, encouragement, discouragement and i barriers from work sources using a six—point scale. The items were then categorized by source (6 items related to supervisor, 3 to union, 8 to coworkers, 3 to the university/organization) and by barrier type (8 items). Total participants’ scores were summed by item and weights were calculated for each category based on the percentage of the total each accounted for (same as the procedure described above). These weights were used in analyzing data from final sample participants. The Work Support/Pressure measure was calculated using the following items and weights (Part IV, Appendix B, items 14 to 19, 21, 24, 25, 27 weighted .026, .026, .028, .021, .030, .028, .039, .032, .084, .043 respectively). Cronbach alpha reliability for this measure based on the final survey 174 responses was .71. The Work Barriers measure was calculated using the following items and weights (items 20, 22, 23, 26, 28, 29 weighted as .037, .029, .030, .055, .029, .057 respectively). Cronbach alpha reliability for the Work Barriers scale was .79. Finally, in order to examine the overall influence of work-related factors on EAP utilization, an overall Work Support/Pressure/Barriers variable was calculated by subtracting the total work barriers score for each participant from his/her total personal support/pressure score. This score was then interpreted as the higher the score the more support and fewer barriers perceived from work sources. EAP Utilization. This dependent variable was a dichotomous variable representing the employee’s utilization or non-utilization of the EAP. This measure was obtained from three sources: (1) EAP records, (2) a question on the survey regarding EAP usage (see Part VIII, Appendix B, question #3), and (3) a series of questions relating to EAP usage to be completed only if employees had used EAP services (Part VIII, Appendix B, questions 4-12). If all three sources indicated the individual had utilized an EAP, the individual was classified as an EAP-user. If all sources did not agree, the individual was classified as a non-user. Other Variables. Demographic information was collected on the participants for additional analyses and information. Employees were asked to provide information regarding their age, gender, education, marital status, income, occupational status, race, and organizational tenure. Analytic Metheds This section describes the statistical procedures used to analyze the hypotheses. Frequency distributions for all items were obtained for the entire sample and by group —7’ 175 (EAP users and non-users). Matrices of Pearson Correlation Coefficients were developed to examine the intercorrelations for the variables in each scale and to assist in examining the discrimination and validity of the scales. Regression and hierarchical multiple regression was utilized to analyze the hypotheses. The multiple correlation coefficient (R) represents the degree to which a dependent variable can be predicted from simultaneous consideration of independent variables. The model and hypotheses will be tested in stages (refer to Figures 10 and 11). Hypotheses 1 to 3 suggest similar predictions regarding the relationship of health history, Health Locus of Control, and social group health values/norms with an individual’s desired health goal. Each hypothesis was stated as a main effect for the specific variable on health goal level and was tested by regressing health goal level on each variable. Support for each hypothesis was indicated by the specific variable explaining a significant amount of variance in desired health goal level. Hypotheses 4a, 4b, 4c and 5 each predicted specific variables (the three Health Locus of Control variables and expectancy of goal attainment) would moderate the relationship between the perceived goal-sensor discrepancy and EAP utilization. Each hypothesis was tested via hierarchical multiple regression where the perceived goal— sensor discrepancy variable was entered in the first step, the particular moderator variable in the second step, and the discrepancy by moderator interaction in the third step. Support for each hypothesis was indicated by a statistically significant beta coefficient corresponding to the interaction variable. Hypotheses 6 and 7 suggest similar predictions regarding the relationship of personal support/pressure/barriers and work support/pressure/barriers with an 7—7 176 individual’s use of an EAP. Each hypothesis was stated as a main effect for the specific variable on EAP utilization and was tested by regressing EAP utilization on each variable. Support for each hypothesis was indicated by the specific variable explaining a significant amount of variance in EAP utilization. Post-hoe analyses were conducted to further examine relationships among the variables and to compare demographic characteristics of EAP users and non—EAP users. if CHAPTER 5 RESULTS Descriptive statistics (Ns, means, and standard deviations) of variables in the model are presented in Table 6. Sample sizes vary across scales because of missing values. lntercorrelations among key variables were also calculated and are shown in Table 7. Few variables are highly correlated with one another. Those that significantly correlate over .40 (at p < .01) worth noting are personal support/pressure to using an EAP with personal barriers (r = .61) and with work barriers (r = .45) (note that these are the individual variables that are used to create the personal and work support/pressure/barriers variables), indicating that those perceiving more personal sources of support/pressure to use an EAP also perceive more barriers. In addition, the personal barriers to using an EAP measure is significantly correlated with work barriers (r = .67) and with using an EAP (r = -.46) so those perceiving more barriers tend to not use an EAP. Work—related support/pressure to use an EAP is negatively related with work—related barriers (r = -.44), indicating those individuals perceiving more support to use an EAP from sources at work (e.g., coworkers, supervisor, union) perceive fewer barriers in the workplace to using an EAP. In general, when the perceived barriers measure is subtracted from the perceived support/pressure measure (both personal and work sources), those perceiving more support/pressure and fewer barriers from personal sources also tend to perceive more support/pressure and fewer barriers from work sources (r = .51). It is also important to note that the correlation (r = -.77) between personal support/pressure/barriers (PSPB in Table 7) and personal barriers (PBAR) is artificially high because of the differential score used to develop the personal 177 178 mm. mm. 0H. mH. mm. wH. mo.m Hm.mm w¢.mm w©.vH mm.v v¢.© me.m Nb.v m@.m .mm mnematzoz new em. mw.v Hb.va bN.mb mo.wvH mm.mv m©.Hv ow.mH mm.wH Nc.©m can: boH boH 50H boH hoH boH boH bcH 56H bcH boH 60H bcH 50H bcH mm mm. om. vH. vH. mH. NH. we.” vw.mm wo.cm mm.om ¢Q.v NH.> VH.m vm.v mm.m I am me. we. NN.H «H.i ow. um. om.v mH.mm mH.Vb Nb.omH mm.mv Hb.Hv wm.mH vo.mH bw.©m awe: acme: ev.v wb.mm mm.v> Hw.wvH em.mv w©.Hv v©.mH mm.mH vm.om can: HHecm>o mbm new mbw new nbN mum mhm new mrm new mhm new new mum new mm wHoHHHam i .Qaam,xgoz chHuaem x963 mnzmmoum\unoan=m x963 mneHccem r .aasm Hazcwnom maoHHme Hmaomgom casemenm\ucoan=w Haaomeem hHonHu :HHmvx aHmm HHHa eoH Hwoo :HHmwm cmeHmw: w£262\mo:Ha> :uHmmm HwHoom cog: cognac cog: «cacao Hsueozom cog: HaepcueH oHanHw> Hobo: one :H onnaHHw> come: no mcoHuwH>oo unwucaum new a all can: "m oHawa 179 mHmocmm - omen mam mcha on weoHucam H. was - mama maoHnon :uHeoz «counse u memo moH Haoo =HHa0= eouHaoo - Hun: HenHaoz Lemmy HHonHa gHHa = - oazo maeoz\mo=Ha> :HHamz HaHoom - z>=m Houueoo Ho maooH auHama manage - zm x=m OO.H ume. **Hm.i ”ebb. ”*H®.t *smm.r bc.t *m—.I umn.t pad. mam: cocH .484.- 448». .asse.- mo. no.- mo.- mo.- *NH. new; OQ.H u9v¢.t subc. *unv. mo. *vn. paw“. *N~.I m=m mama mama «an: mam: m acme: wcon< acoHuuHmanoououcH '1‘ is «Haas 180 support/pressure/barriers measure-i.e., the personal barriers measure is used as one variable to calculate the personal support/pressure/barriers measure. Similarly, correlations are artificially high between work support/pressure/barriers (WSPB) and the two sources used to develop this variable (work support/pressure or WSUPP with r = .77, and work barriers or WBAR with r = -.91). The correlation between current problems and desired change (i.e., perceived discrepancy) is also significant (r = —.63) where those who have more health problems (a low score on the current problems scale) tend to perceive more of a discrepancy between their desired and current health (a high score on the desired change scale). Finally, as previously mentioned, a health goal-health status discrepancy measure was calculated to use as a method check to determine whether the model and relationships exist as predicted within the control theory framework. This health goal- health status discrepancy variable was calculated by standardizing each employee’s scores on both the desired health goal and current health problems measures. These scores for each employee were then compared to determine whether the scores fell within the same percentile-i.e., if an employee’s standardized score on desired health goal was in the Upper 25th percentile and the person’s standardized score on current health problems was also in the upper 25th percentile of scores then no discrepancy exists. If both scores for an individual did not fall within the same percentile, a health goal-health status discrepancy existed. Once all participants’ scores were calculated, a t—test was conducted using the “perceived discrepancy” measure as the dependent variable to examine whether a significant difference was found between individuals without a discrepancy and those with one (with individuals categorized with/without a discrepancy based on standardized scores). The t-test indicated a significant difference between the two groups 181 (p = .02). As a method check on the control theory model, this calculated health goal— health status discrepancy measure (called the ”actual" discrepancy) was also correlated with the "perceived” discrepancy measure created by the desired change variable (r = .13, p < .05). To determine whether the relationships within the model might exist as predicted, these two variables should be significantly correlated. The correlation between the variables was found to be significant but weak. Thus, additional research may be required to assess how useful control theory may be as a framework for understanding the EAP decision process. Hypothesis # 1 The first set of hypotheses (#1, #2 and #3) suggested similar predictions regarding the relationship ofa specific independent variable with an individual’s desired health goal. Each hypothesis was tested by regressing desired health goal level on the particular variable. Hypothesis #1 suggests that an individual’s past health (health history) will influence his/her desired health goal level. A main effect was predicted for health history such that individuals with more prior health problems would demonstrate a higher desired health goal level than individuals with fewer prior health problems. This hypothesis was tested by regressing desired health goal on health history. A significant amount of variance in the desired health goal level was not explained by health history (R2=.001, n.s., n=274); thus, support for the hypothesis is not indicated. The correlation between health history and health goal level was also not significant (r = .02, 11.8.). 182 11112941393315.1912. Hypothesis #2 predicted that an individual’s Health Locus of Control (HLOC) will have an effect on the person’s desired health goal level such that individuals who are internals will have a higher desired health goal level than those who are externals (i.e., score higher on either the powerful others or chance subscale). To test this, a separate regression was conducted on each of the three HLOC subscales. Table 8 displays the results of these regressions. Internal HLOC was not a significant predictor of desired health goal. The correlation was also not significant (1' = .07, n.s.). Similarly, the Powerful Others HLOC subscale was not a significant predictor. However, the Chance HLOC subscale was a significant predictor of desired health goal level. In addition, Chance HLOC correlated negatively with an individual’s desired health goal level, and the correlation between these variables was significant (r = -.235. P < ~01)- Hypothesis # 3 Hypothesis #3 suggested that an individual’s perception of the health values and norms of members of his/her social group would affect the individual’s desired health goal such that individuals who perceived more positive health norms among family and friends, past and present, would hold higher health goals than those who perceived lower health norms among their family and friends. This hypothesis was tested by regressing desired health goal on the social health values/norms measure. The regression results indicate that support for this hypothesis was not found (R2=.004, n.s., n =274). 183 Table 8: Regression Results for the Health Locus of Control (HLOC) Subscales on Desired Health Goal Level Regression Results for the Internal Health Locus of Control (HLOC) Subscale on Desired Health Goal Level Variable B R R2 p of R2 Internal HLOC .084 .071 .005 n.s. Regression Results for the Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (HLOC) Subscale on Desired Health Goal Level Powerful Others HLOC -.052 .051 .005 n.s. Regression Results for the Chance Health Locus of Control (HLOC) Subscale on Desired Health Goal Level Chance HLOC -.217 .235 .055 .0001** * p < .05 ** p < .01 E4mE . _ 184 Hypotheses #4afia#4b}and #4c The fourth set of hypotheses stated that the Health Locus of Control variables would moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. Hypothesis # 4a predicted that individuals who are high on the Internal HLOC subscale would respond to a perceived discrepancy between their desired and current health status by utilizing an EAP more than those who are low on the Internal HLOC subscale. Hypothesis #4b predicted that individuals who are low on the Powerful Others HLOC subscale would respond to a perceived discrepancy between their desired and current health status by utilizing EAP services more compared to those who are high on this scale. Similarly, hypothesis #4c predicted that individuals who are low on the Chance HLOC subscale would respond to a perceived discrepancy between their desired and current health status by utilizing an EAP more than those who are high on this scale. Thus, it was predicted that those individuals who were internals (i.e., high on Internal HLOC) would respond to their perceived discrepancy by utilizing an EAP more than individuals who were externals (i.e., high on Powerful Others or Chance HLOC). These hypotheses were tested by running separate moderated regression analyses for each of the Health Locus of Control subscales. EAP usage was regressed on perceived discrepancy, each of the Health Locus of Control subscales, and the perceived discrepancy by Health Locus of Control subscale interaction term. These results are displayed in Table 9 and plotted in Figures 14 to 16. The interaction term for Internal HLOC by perceived discrepancy was marginally significant (at the p < .10 level). The interaction term for Chance HLOC by perceived discrepancy was also significant (p < .05). However, the interaction of Powerful Others HLOC by perceived discrepancy was 185 Table 9: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Health Locus of Control Subscales, Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction Hypothesis #4a: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Internal Health Locus of Control (HLOC), Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction Hierarchical 2 2 Step Variable Beta R Adj R Change R 1 Perceived Discrepancy .682 .022 —.003 .001 2 Internal HLOC .417 .105 .004 .011* 3 Perceived Discrepancy x Internal HLOC ~.732 .144 .001 .001* Hypothesis #4b: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (HLOC), Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction Hierarchical 2 Step Variable Beta R Adj R Change R2 1 Perceived Discrepancy *.263 .022 —.003 .001 2 Powerful Others HLOC ~.352 .130 .010* .017** 3 Perceived Discrepancy x Powerful Others HLOC .386 .154 .013* .007 HYpothesis #4c: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Chance Health Locus of Control (HLOC), Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction —-¥ Hierarchical 2 Step Variable Beta R Adj R Change R2 1 Perceived Discrepancy -.399 .022 —.003 .001 2 Chance HLOC -.410 .033 -.006 .001 3 Perceived Discrepancy X Chance HLOC .556 .136 .008 .018** * p < .10 ** p < .05 186 Hypothesis #4a Yes High IHLC EAP Utilization No Low IHLC 50 63 75 88 100 Perceived Discrepancy Figure 14: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Internal Health Locus of Control (IHLC) 187 Hypothesis #4b Yes Low PHLC High PHLC BAP Utilization N0 50 63 75 88 100 Perceived Discrepancy Figure 15: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Powerful Others Health Locus of Control (PHLC) 188 Hypothesis #40 Yes Low CHLC EAP Utilization High CHLC No 50 63 75 88 100 Perceived Discrepancy Figure 16: Results of Regressing BAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Chance Health Locus of Control (CHLC) 189 not significant. Moderator effects for Internal and Chance HLOC were found, thus hypotheses #4a and #4c were supported. Hypothesis # 5 Hypothesis # 5 predicted that an individual’s expectancy regarding the ability of the EAP to resolve or improve his/her current health problems would moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. It was predicted that individuals who are high on the expectancy measure will respond to a perceived discrepancy between current and desired health by utilizing an EAP more compared to individuals who are low on the expectancy scale. Employees’ EAP usage was regressed on perceived discrepancy, expectancy, and the perceived discrepancy by expectancy interaction term. Table 10 and Figure 17 display the regression results which indicate that expectancy is not significant as a moderator between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. Hypothesis #6 This hypothesis predicted a main effect for personal support/pressure/barriers to seeking EAP assistance such that individuals who perceive higher levels of support/pressure and fewer barriers from personal sources will tend to have used an EAP for services more compared to individuals who report low levels of support/pressure and perceive more personal barriers to seeking EAP assistance. This hypothesis was tested by regressing the personal support/pressure/barriers measure on EAP utilization. A significant amount of variance in EAP utilization was explained by this variable, thus the hypothesis is supported (R2 = .158, P < -01, D = 274). 190 Table 10: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Expectancy, Perceived Discrepancy, and the Interaction Hierarchical Step Variable Beta R Adj R2 Change R2 1 Perceived Discrepancy «.118 .026 -.003 .001 2~ Expectancy —.635 .031“ —.006 .000 3 Perceived Discrepancy x Expectancy .567 .047 —.009 .001 * p < .10 ** p < .05 191 Hypothesis 5 Yes High Exp'y EAP Low Exp'y Utilization No 50 63 75 88 100 Perceived Discrepancy Figure 17: Results of Regressing'EAP Utilization on Perceived Discrepancy by Expectancy 192 Hypothesis # 7 Hypothesis # 7 suggested a main effect for work-related support/pressure/barriers to seeking EAP assistance such that individuals who perceive higher levels of support/pressure and fewer barriers from sources within the organization will tend to have used an EAP for services more compared to individuals who report low levels of support/pressure and perceive more work-related barriers to seeking EAP assistance. This hypothesis was tested by regressing the work support/pressure/barriers measure on BAP utilization. A significant amount of variance in EAP utilization was explained by this variable, thus the hypothesis is supported (R2 = .058, p < .01, n = 274). The work support/pressure/barriers variable was calculated by subtracting an individual’s perception of work—related barriers to using an EAP from his/her perception of work support/pressure to use an EAP. The correlation between this new variable created called work support/pressure/barriers and EAP utilization was significant (r = .24, p < .01). The work—related barriers variable alone was also significantly correlated with EAP utilization (r = -.294, p < .01). The correlation between work support/pressure and EAP utilization was not significant (r = .074, n.s.). Post-Hoc Analyses Most past EAP research has examined demographic variables and EAP utilization to understand the profile of EAP users. The purpose of the present research was to examine additional factors which potentially may influence EAP utilization. These additional variables might be classified into two categories: those that are related to perceived support, pressures, and barriers to using EAP services and those that are related to personal attitudes, characteristics, and beliefs. A post-hoc analysis was 193 conducted to examine whether the two additional categories of variables significantly added to our understanding of EAP utilization beyond demographic characteristics. To do this, a hierarchical regression was conducted where demographic variables were entered in the first step, the support/pressure/barriers variables were entered in the second step, and the attitudes and belief variables were entered in the third step. Results of this regression are displayed in Table 11. The regression indicates that demographics had no significant influence on EAP utilization (R2 change = .039, n.s., n = 262) while the addition of the personal and work support/pressure/barriers variables had a significant impact on EAP usage (R2 change = .175, p < .01, n = 262). Also, adding the attitudes and beliefs variables (Health Locus of Control and Expectancy) significantly added to explaining EAP utilization beyond the demographic and support/pressure/barriers variables (R2 change == .008, n.s., n == 262). This regression indicates that Health Locus of Control and the expectancy that an EAP will help resolve problems may be key variables influencing EAP utilization along with perceptions of personal and work-related sources of support/pressure and barriers to utilizing EAPs. A few additional variables measured on all participants were examined next and individuals were again compared by EAP-usage and non-usage. Employees were asked whether they had the opportunity to utilize affordable assistance services other than the organization’s EAP. This question was asked separately to examine the relationship between having other resources available and EAP utilization. The potential availability of other resources was not included in the personal support/pressure or barriers measures described above because these resources were not viewed as a potential source 0f support nor as a potential barrier to EAP usage. While it might be argued by some that the availability of other resources might decrease EAP usage, the present research 194 Table 11: Results of Regressing EAP Utilization on Demographic Variables, Support/Pressure/Barriers Variables, and Personal Bel iefs/Value Variables Hierarchical , Step Variable Beta R Adj R2 Change R2 1 Demographics .197 .008 .039 " Tenure .042 Job Position .106 Race —.021 Marital Status —.057 Gender .122 Age —.082 Education - -.004 Family Income ~.007 2 Support/Pressure/Barriers .462 .182** .175** Personal .409 Work .029 3 Health Locus of Control and Expectancy .471 .178** .008** Internal HLOC .056 Powerful Others HLOC -.029 Chance HLOC .027 Expectancy —.067 **p< .01 195 did not view EAP utilization in this manner. EAP services include the referral of clients to external resources, thus EAPs typically operate in an environment where a variety of other assistance services are available and EAPs are often viewed by clients as the first source of assistance. A t—test was conducted examining the differences in the means between EAP users and non-users on this variable. This test was significant (p < .05) and indicated that EAP users also perceived more access to other resources. A crosstabulation analysis was performed between EAP usage and perceived opportunity to use other services, and results are presented in Table 12. Those who perceive an opportunity to utilize other assistance services (e.g., spouse’s EAP, church counselors, psychiatrist) still tend to take greater advantage of the organization’s EAP services. Also, those who do not use the organization’s EAP also do not perceive there are other services available. Individuals were also asked about their willingness to use the organization’s EAP if the services were needed. A t-test was conducted comparing the means of the EAP users and non-users on this variable and the t—test was significant (p < .01). While it may n0t be surprising to find that EAP users tend to be more willing to use an EAP in the future compared with non-users, it is encouraging to find that the experience of using an EAP did not turn EAP users against future utilization. . . a 196 Table 12: Crosstabulation Analysis of Availability of Other Resources and EAP Utilization Total Non-EAP Usage EAP Usage Chi-Square Variable N N % N % Significance Other Resources .024** Yes 150 49 .33 101 .67 % of N by Usage .46 .60 No 124 57 .46 67 .54 % of N by Usage .53 .40 * p < .05 ** p < .01 CHAPTER 6 DISCUSSION The purpose of the present research was to examine factors which may influence an employee’s decision to seek EAP assistance and to develop a conceptual model to guide research in this area. A Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization was developed and parts of the model were tested in order to gain a better understanding of the EAP utilization process. The setting was an organization which provided a number of EAP services to all employees as part of its employee benefits package. All employees in the sample had equal access to the EAP. Seven main hypotheses were studied. Three hypotheses examined factors influencing the goal/standard variable, i.e., the desired health goal, within the control theory model. None of these hypotheses was fully supported. In addition, two hypotheses predicted a moderator effect on the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization for Health Locus of Control and expectancy that an EAP will help improve one’s health problems. Health Locus of Control was found to moderate this relationship, though the hypothesis was not completely supported. The last two hypotheses tested for a direct relationship with EAP utilization for the following two variables: personal support/pressure/barriers and work support/pressure/barriers. Both variables were found to be significantly related to usage. The following sections will provide a discussion of these findings. H. ypothesis fi 1; Effect of Health History on Desired Health Goal Hypothesis #1 predicted that an individual’s health history would significantly affect his/her desired health goal level such that the more health problems an individual 197 198 has experienced, the more likely he/she would hold higher health goals. The regression analysis found no significant relationship between health history and desired health goal, thus this hypothesis was not supported. There are a number of potential reasons for the failure to observe a significant relationship between a person’s past health experiences and his/her desired health goal. First, there may be problems with the measurement of the health goal construct. For example, it may be difficult to obtain variability among individuals when measuring what they desire their health to be because how many individuals would desire or admit to desiring poor health? A second possible explanation for the lack of an observed relationship is that the expectations for the effect may have been overly Optimistic. Since there was no previous research found that suggested a relationship might exist between these two variables, the hypothesis was based on an inferred relationship. This relationship assumed that poor health would lead to a heightened desire for good health, and thus to higher health goals. An alternative explanation might be that the lack of a relationship suggests that some individuals with poor health may actually want to try to improve their health and desire higher health goals while others with poor health may become more realistic about their health status and actually feel they will have to learn to live with their problems. Thus, due to their past health experiences, individuals may actually lower their health goals to what may be more realistic health expectations. It could be that no relationship between past health and desired health goal was observed due to the two potential responses among individuals that may have cancelled each other out. Research on goal—setting may further help to explain the lack of an observed relationship. Campion and Lord (1982) have stated that the literature indicates that past 199 performance and ability are primary determinants of initial goal levels, which then serve as referents for future behavior. Subsequent discrepancies between feedback and this referent goal creates a motivation to reduce the discrepancy. However, depending on individual or situational characteristics, a person may reduce the discrepancy by modifying either his/her behavior or goals (Campion & Lord, 1982). Campion and Lord found that past success and high ability tend to lead to higher goal levels. Hollenbeck and Williams (1987) also concluded that those individuals setting the highest goal levels held perceptions of high past performance levels. However, others examining the goal- setting process have reported that some individuals raised their goals after failure while others lowered their goals following success (Kernan & Lord, 1985). Two additional variables that have been found to influence the goal-setting process may explain the ambiguity. These are goal importance and self-focus. According to Carver and Scheier (1981), a person’s attention can be directed in one of two directions: inward toward the self or outward toward the environment. When attention is inward, the individual is engaging in self-focus or self-attention. Hollenbeck and Williams (1987) found that individuals high in self-focus are more aware of the discrepancy between goals and feedback, and thus are more likely to undertake discrepancy-reducing behavior compared to individuals who are low in self-focus. They also found that the setting of a goal is an interactive function of perceptions of one’s past performance, self—focus, and perceived goal importance where the more successful the performance and the higher the level of self-focus and goal importance, the more the individual sets higher goal levels. Future research should examine the influence of self- focus and goal importance on desired health goal level. 200 Another variable that may be important in understanding the goalesetting process is self—efficacy. Self—efficacy refers to the judgments people make about their ability to execute courses of action, where those who are high in self—efficacy feel they can master some task. It differs from Internal Health Locus of Control because those high in Internal HLOC believe their actions are responsible for outcomes received but having an Internal HLOC does not necessarily mean the person believes he/she has the ability to actually execute the actions to obtain desired outcomes (Bandura, 1982). Individuals high in self-efficacy tend to perceive they have the ability to execute the actions. Self- efficacy is not the same as believing one has control, although. there may be a relationship between Health Locus of Control and self-efficacy (it seems logical that individuals who have an internal health locus might also be high in self-efficacy, although the relationship between self-efficacy and external locus of control is uncertain). Self-efficacy has been found to be related to a person’s past accomplishments where past successes increase feelings of self-efficacy if attributable to unchanging factors, like personal ability, while past failures tend to reduce these feelings (Bandura, 1982). If individuals have been relatively healthy in the past (health success), their self-efficacy may increase, depending on what they attribute the cause of their success to. These individuals may increase or hold a high desired health goal level because they believe they have the ability to carry out their tasks/goals. On the other hand, if individuals have had poor health in the past, their self—efficacy may be lower, and thus they may tend to hold lower health goals because they fail to perceive they have the ability to achieve these goals anyway. Thus, self-efficacy may be an additional individual characteristic that might interact with past history to determine future goal 201 levels. Future research might examine the relationship among self-efficacy, prior health status, and health goals. Hypothesis #2: Effect of Health Locus of Control on Desired Health Goal In the second hypothesis, individuals with an Internal HLOC were predicted to hold a higher desired health goal level compared to individuals with an External HLOC (either Powerful Others or Chance). There was no statistically significant relationship found between desired health goal level and Internal HLOC or between desired health goal level and Powerful Others HLOC (an external subscale). However, the regression analysis found a statistically significant relationship between desired health goal level and individuals’ beliefs about the extent their health is a function of luck or fate-—i.e., Chance HLOC (External). Thus, while the hypothesis is not fully supported, it is interesting to note that those individuals who believed their health was determined by chance also held lower desired health goals. After all, belief in chance suggests the individual has no control over his/her health. It is interesting to note that the present research provides more explanation on why individuals lower their health goals but few guidelines as to why individuals may be more health conscious. While chance may be associated with a reduction in desired health goals, no explanation was provided in the research as to factors that enhance an individual’s health goals. Also, a potential problem in observing an effect, as previously discussed, is that it may be difficult to obtain adequate variation in the desired health goal level. Being healthy is such a desired state that little variation may exist in the population regarding this factor. 202 Past research on Health Locus of Control has examined its relationship with actual healthy and health-seeking behaviors. The relationship between Health Locus of Control and what one desires, i.e., desired health goals, has not been previously examined. Also, no research was found on health decisions and the difference between what an individual desires and what the person actually does. Thus, further research needs to be conducted to understand personality factors which may be involved. Certainly it’s not unique for there to be a discrepancy between what people say and what they do. Hypothesis #3: Effect of Social Group Health Values/Norms on Desired Health Goal Hypothesis #3 predicted a relationship between an individual’s desired health goal level and the health values and norms held by members of the individual’s social group. It was hypothesized that the more positive the perceived health values/norms are of one’s social group members, the higher the individual’s desired health goal level. Similar to Mayer’s (1988) conclusion that health norms within an organizational setting had no relationship with an employee’s health attitudes and behaviors and Hung’s (1988) finding that EAP use was not significantly related to perceptions of the organization’s "climate" (warm or cool toward employees), the regression analysis in the present study found no significant relationship between health norms/values of social group members and the individual’s health goals. However, this finding is contrary to past research that has concluded that sick-role/healthy attitudes and behaviors are, at least partly, learned from others (Bruhn & Cordova, 1977; Mechanic, 1978; Pender, 1982; Rosenstock, 1975). This is an area that may require additional clarification. i 203 One explanation for the lack of a relationship may be that there are perhaps two effects going on simultaneously which would tend to cancel each other out. For example, a person’s social ties may result in one of two responses. Social relationships have been found to help reduce stress levels among individuals with close relationships (Cassel, 1976; Hirsch, 1980; Kaplan & Cassel, 1977). In addition, Lin, Simeone, Ensel and Kuo (1979) found that social support contributed significantly and negatively to illness symptoms such that individuals with more social support experienced fewer symptoms. Therefore, if social ties are close, individuals may actually experience fewer health problems/symptoms (Eaton, 1978; Hirsch, 1980; Lin, Simeone, Ensel & Kuo, 1979). In addition, close social ties have been found to encourage individuals to be more positive and proactive in their health attitudes and behaviors when they feel they have problems (Haskell & Blair, 1980; House, 1981). It thus seems that an important factor influencing attitudes and behaviors is that individuals must perceive a need or problem. On the other hand, if social ties are weak or disrupted, the influence of the members of one’s social group on an individual may actually increase the person’s susceptibility and perception of illness (Pilisuk & Minkler, 1985). Thus, the perception of the norms and values of social group members may result in different responses depending on the closeness of the individual members. The actual relationship might be as follows: Social Group Health Values/Norms > Health History ----- > Desired- Health Goal Level. If the two responses discussed were occurring simultaneously, then no effect would have been found between social group health values/norms and desired health goal level or between health status and desired health goal level (hypothesis # 1). Another potential problem is that in the present research, social health norms/values were examined regarding past social group members (parents, family, 204 friends growing up) as well as present members (spouse, children, etc.) and these perceptions were formed into one measure. Current levels of ”closeness" to past/present social group members were not measured in the present research, nor was the present "quality" of the relationships measured; thus, positive norms of social group members may exist or have existed, but depending on whether these members are still an important aspect of an individual’s life could determine the degree of influence the norms/values currently have on individuals. In further examining the relationship between social group health values/norms and desired health goal, the correlation between these two variables was also found to not be significant. However, it is interesting to note the variables with which the social group health values/norms measure was found to be significantly correlated. For example, the social group health values/norms measure was significantly correlated with the number of current health problems reported by an individual (r = .23, p < .01) as well as the expectancy than an EAP will help resolve these problems (r = .13, p < .05). This might suggest that, while peer pressure/norms may not affect an individual’s health goals, these social influences could be related to the person’s perception of his/her current health such that individuals who perceive more positive health norms of social group members (i.e., the more health conscious the social group is perceived) also tend to perceive more health problems in themselves and to believe the EAP will help resolve these problems. Thus, a person’s social group may be influencing his/her perceptions when the person compares his/her own health to the health and health-consciousness of his/her social group. Therefore, although the regression analysis suggests there is no significant relationship between social group health values/norms and desired health goal 205 level, correlationally an individual’s perceptions of the health values/norms of social group members appear to be related to other issues. The issue of how one’s social group influences an individual’s desired health standard/goal is an important concern within organizations. With the amount of effort and money being spent by firms to encourage employees to be more health conscious, a greater understanding of the role of peer pressure and social group norms is required. We don’t know enough about these influences, and, in fact, there were contradictory findings in the manner others may influence individuals’ health behavior. For example, in the present research Powerful Others Health Locus of Control had no influence on what an individual wanted regarding his/her health (desired health goal). However, when an individual perceived his/her social group to be health conscious, then the individual tended to report more health problems. This suggests that on the one hand there may be a relationship between perceptions of the health consciousness of one’s social group and the health problems reported by that same individual. However, when individuals specifically perceive others are in control of them (i.e., their health) then these others may have less of an impact on the person. The interplay between what a person sees others doing (being health conscious) and its effect on the person and the effect others have on individuals when they are perceived to be in control of the person needs further study. Summ r of Rel tionshi s with Desir H al h Goal It could be concluded that an individual’s perception of his/her health history, perceiving being in control of his/her health (Internal HLOC) and the health values and norms of one’s social group did not influence the individual’s desired health goal level 206 in this study. However, while prior health problems and feeling in control over one’s health may not have affected the person’s health goal, believing chance controls the person’s health may be an important factor because the person who believes that chance is the controlling factor tends to hold lower health goals. Also, if individuals that the person associates with are health conscious, these individuals may influence the person to perceive that he/she is not as healthy (and thus report more health problems, or possibly influence the person to do something to improve his/her health, such as seeking EAP assistance). On the one hand, it may be helpful to understand how the belief in chance/fate/luck controlling one’s health might operate'to influence a person’s health goal. The suggestion in the present research that the belief in chance controlling one’s health might influence a person to lower his/her health goals is intriguing. For instance, could the relationship between Chance Health Locus of Control and desired health goal level be influenced by a person justifying or rationalizing his/her health beliefs and behaviors? Might a person believe that chance controls his/her health because it’s a good excuse to not have to work toward good health? For example, how often does a person who smokes say “I know smoking causes cancer but it won’t happen to me" or say ”it doesn’t matter what I do, if I’m supposed to get sick I will”? Thus, a person can justify smoking or other unhealthy behaviors and thus not have to make any changes in his/her life since ”it won’t do any good anyway” because the person believes his/her health is based on luck (or chance). On the other hand, future research examining influences on the desired health goal level may not produce a greater understanding of EAP utilization behavior. Given the fact that the variables hypothesized to influence desired health goals were not ii 207 significantly related to the desired health goal level, along with the finding that an individual’s desired health goal level was not correlated with EAP utilization, it may be . that health goals do not significantly impact EAP utilization. However, as previously suggested, the lack of an observed relationship with desired health goal level may have been due to the operationalization of the desired health goal construct in the present study. One criticism of the measure is that it failed to discriminate among individuals holding high or low desired health goals. The range of scores that was possible for the desired health goal level measure was 11 to 58. In the present research, scores ranged from 24 to 56, with a mean and median of 44 and a standard deviation of 4.68. Over 84 percent of the respondents scored over 40 and less than one percent scored under 30. When comparing the scores from the top and bottom third of the respondents, the lower third scored 42 or less while the upper third scored 46 or more. It may be that individuals will not vary greatly in the extent they desire to be healthy, or not be consciously aware of a lower desired health goal level. Another potential problem with the measure used is that the questions were worded in an absolute sense so that it was difficult to tell if a person’s desire to be in good health was the same whether expressed by a healthy or less healthy individual. For example, individuals were asked what their desired health level was compared to others their age. Over 82 percent reported they desired above average or way above average health level and less than two percent desired to be less than average. In another example, individuals were asked how many pounds within their ideal weight they would like to fall. Over 43 percent desired to be within two to five pounds of their ideal weight and a total of 77 percent responded they desired to be within ten pounds of their ideal weight. Only 8 percent reported they desired to be 25 pounds or over 25 pounds from 208 their ideal weight. The wording of the questions did not make it possible to differentiate among the different degrees of desired health. An individual who weighed 350 pounds and desired to be within 30 pounds of his/her ideal weight would be classified as desiring to be less healthy than the person who weighs 140 pounds but desires to be within ten pounds of his/her ideal weight. It may be necessary to develop a measure of desired health that is corrected for the absolute level on the variable to adequately distinguish among different desired health goal levels. A final issue regarding the health goal measure may involve whether individuals consciously hold specific health goals, or at least goals that go beyond simply a "general desire to be healthy." How behavior and goals are influenced is a complicated matter. For example, one factor predicted to be central to EAP utilization behavior is the perceived discrepancy variable (in the present study this is the perceived discrepancy between desired and current health). The remaining hypotheses focus on factors which were predicted to influence actual help—seeking behavior regarding EAP usage. Hypgtheses #4a, #4b and fi4c: The Moderator Influence of Health Locus of Control The fourth set of hypotheses predicted that Health Locus of Control would moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. In other words, it was predicted that the strength of the relationship between the difference between desired and current health and EAP utilization will be stronger for those individuals who are high on Internal HLOC than it will be for those with who have a low Internal HLOC. It was also predicted that the strength of the relationship between the difference between desired and current health and EAP utilization will be weaker for those individuals who are high on either of the External HLOC subscales (Powerful 209 Others or Chance) than it will be for those with who are lower on either of these subscales. The regression analysis indicated that Internal HLOC and Chance HLOC were both significant moderators between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. Hypotheses # 4a and # 4c were supported in that those with an Internal HLOC did tend to use the EAP while those believing luck controlled their health tended to not use the EAP. However, Powerful Others HLOC had no significant moderator effect, just as it had no significant direct effect on desired health goal (see hypothesis #2). In other words, the belief that powerful others control an individual’s health had no significant relationship with the individual’s health goals or health behavior (EAP usage). It is interesting to find that Internal HLOC was significantly related to a person’s EAP use but was not related to the person’s desired health goal (see hypothesis #2). Prior research on HLOC compared internals and externals on a specific behavior—- smoking/stop smoking, weight loss, use of seat belts or birth control, etc.--and has typically found internals to be more proactive. In the second hypothesis, however, HLOC was examined in its relationship to something desired (goal) not an actual behavior undertaken. It is possible that there might be a difference between a health goal, which is what one desires, and using an EAP (hypothesis #4), which is what one actually does. On the other hand, it is also possible that the lack of a relationship with desired health goal level was due to the operationalization of the construct, as previously discussed. Another explanation as to why individuals who believe chance controls their health do not tend to use an EAP might be that those who believe chance controls their health simply see no need to undertake such a behavior (going to an EAP) in order to influence their health. This explanation is consistent with the relationships reported in 210 the present research. For example, Chance HLOC was found to be negatively related to the belief that an EAP will help (r = -.18, p < .01) and positively related to the number of barriers perceived to using an EAP from both personal and work-related sources (r = .14, p < .05 for both personal barriers and work barriers). In addition, Chance HLOC was also negatively correlated with perceived current health problems (r = -.21, p < .01), thus believing that chance controls one’s health is related to an individual perceiving fewer health problems. The regression analysis suggested that Powerful Others HLOC had no impact on EAP utilization. Previous research has typically found that individuals with an External HLOC (including Powerful Others HLOC) tend to take less control over their health behaviors--e.g., External HLOC individuals don’t tend to quit smoking or lose weight, "external" cystic fibrosis patients don’t tend to keep doctor appointments-because presumably they believe their behaviors will not impact their health ((Best, 1975; Coan, 1973; James, Woodruff, & Werner, 1972; O’Bryan, 1972; Platt,. 1969; Schultz, 1981). However, regarding some health behaviors the findings have been equivocal. For example, Bellack, Rozensky and Schwartz (1974) found no significant relation between weight loss and locus of control. The present analysis indicated that usage and non- usage of an EAP occurred by both employees who scored high and employees who scored low on the Powerful Others HLOC subscale. In the present research the correlation between Powerful Otheis HLOC and the belief or expectancy that an EAP will improve one’s health was also not significant. The lack of a relationship between Powerful Others HLOC and EAP utilization might be due to the low reliability of the Powerful Others HLOC subscale in the present study. While Wallston and Wallston (1978) previously found reliability for this subscale 211 to be .71 (Cronbach Alpha), results from the third pilot test conducted for the present research indicated a reliability of .67 while results from the actual study data indicated a reliability of only .58. Also, in examining the data, almost 67% of the employees were classified as "low" on this measure and only .4% (one person) were classified as "high" (refer to the "Measures” section in Chapter 4 on Health Locus of Control). Thus, there may also be a restriction of range problem on this measure. Another explanation is that individuals who tend to hold a Powerful Others HLOC have been found to be more susceptible to external or social pressures (Saltzer,1978). For some, social pressures may be viewed as a form of support. House ( 1981) has suggested three ways in which support may reduce the impact of stress on an individual: it may reduce the importance of the perception that a situation is stressful, it may tranquilize the individual’s system (neuroendocrine system) so people are less reactive to perceived stress, and it may facilitate health behaviors. Consequently, for some individuals, believing that others control one’s health may be viewed as a form of support so that when a "powerful" person in the individual’s life encourages/supports the individual in seeking help, the individual tends to seek assistance. For others, control by Powerful Others may be viewed as a form of social support that may tend to actually reduce the number of perceived health problems. For instance, as previously mentioned, Lin, Simeone, Ensel and Kuo (1979) found that social support contributed significantly and negatively to illness symptoms such that individuals with more social support experienced fewer symptoms. Eaton (1978) studied life events, social support and psychiatric symptoms and concluded that social support among household members helps prevent mental disorders. Hirsch (1980) compared individuals who had recently been divorced and found that support enhances 212 one’s adaptation to stress. Cassel (1976) and Kaplan and Cassel (1977) concluded that social support may actually ameliorate the effects of stress for individuals and, thus, reduce the need for treatment. It is possible that feeling others are controlling or concerned about one’s health may act as a buffer and protect individuals from feeling under as much stress and in need of some form of assistance. Thus, Powerful Others HLOC may have failed to have a significant impact on EAP usage because two responses might be possible for individuals who believe others control their health. These individuals may see the situation as less in need of their seeking assistance because others are taking care of them or because others reduce the impact of their problem, or individuals may perceive more support and encouragement to do something about their health problem as being positive and thus may tend to follow the advice of these powerful others in their lives. To further explore why Powerful Others HLOC did not seem to significantly affect what a person desires (health goals) or what the person does (seek EAP assistance) when a problem is perceived, an examination of the intercorrelations between Powerful Others HLOC and other key variables was undertaken. Powerful Others HLOC was found to be significantly correlated with: the number of reported past health problems/health history (r = .23, p < .01) ), reported current health problems (r = -.16, p < .01), and EAP utilization (r = -.l3, p < .05). It is interesting to note the direction of the relationships. Believing that powerful others have control over a person’s health is related to an increase in the number of health problems the individual reports he/she experienced in the past but to a decrease in the number of health problems the individual reports he/she currently has. 213 While the negative relationship between Powerful Others HLOC and current health problems does not allow us to make a causal conclusion, the relationship might suggest that believing powerful others control one’s health may actually be viewed as a form of support that might influence (reduce) the current number of perceived health problems. The negative relationship might also suggest some form of denial process. It could be argued that believing others control an individual’s health may influence the problems reported but may have less influence on whether the person will do something about it--i.e., go to an EAP (since the individual reports fewer current problems). We need to better understand the nature of Health Locus of Control and how perceptions of who or what controls health (Internal, Powerful Others, or Chance) impact health attitudes and behaviors. A focus of future research could be to explore this relationship between powerful others and chance. Since the data indicate that both are related (correlationally) to an individual’s reporting fewer health problems, the influence of external factors on an individual’s decision making process regarding health-related matters is obviously a complicated phenomenon that deserves more study. Perhaps efforts designed to encourage more health-related behaviors need to consider the importance of first making the individual think he/she is in control of his/her health before others can influence (tell) the individual what to do. H the is # ' Th Moderator Influence of Ex ectanc It was hypothesized that an individual’s belief that an EAP will help reduce/resolve current health problems identified by the individual (referred to as the "expectancy" that an EAP will help) would moderate the relationship between perceived discrepancy and EAP utilization. lf individuals perceive a discrepancy between their 214 current and desired health and believe that the EAP will help reduce this discrepancy, it was predicted that the individuals would be more likely to go to an EAP. However, if individuals perceive a discrepancy but do not believe the EAP will help, then they will tend to not use the EAP. In the moderated regression analysis, an individual’s expectancy that an EAP will improve one’s health was not found to be a significant moderator between an individual’s perception of a health discrepancy and EAP utilization. The zero-order correlation between expectancy and EAP utilization was also not significant (r = -.02, n.s.). Expectancies have been found to be important in determining whether behavior will be initiated, and the amount of effort exerted and sustained over time (Bandura, 1977b, 1982). However, it has also been recognized that human beings do not go through rational patterns when making decisions about their behavior (Swanson, 1972). Irrational emotions and motivations are also driving forces in health behavior (DiMarco, 1985). Knowledge about one’s health and belief in treatment effectiveness to improve it do not ensure one will act wisely, due to emotions and motives (DiMarco, 1985; Moriyano, Kreuger & Stamler, 1971). According to Rotter (1954), the occurrence of a behavior is a function of the individual’s expectancy that the given behavior will secure a reinforcement and the value of that reinforcement. An individual seeks to maximize positive rewards or reinforcements (Rotter, Chance & Phares, 1972). Thus, while an individual may believe an EAP can help improve his/her health, the person may not feel compelled or motivated enough to actually seek treatment. Bandura and Walters (1963) suggest that for behavior to occur in a given situation, it must be available to the person and must have been reinforced during previous learning experiences. Thus, individuals who have a positive or high expectancy that an EAP will help and believe a high 215 incentive or reinforcement exists may tend to seek treatment while others who have a high expectancy may not necessarily perceive enough incentive or reinforcement to act. To better understand why expectancy was not a significant variable in the present study, a post hoc analysis was conducted to examine the relationship between expectancy and other major variables within the model. The correlation between expectancy and Chance HLOC was significant and negative (r = -.18, p < .01). Thus, it could be that the more an individual believed his/her health was due to chance (and less to internal factors) the less the person believed the EAP would help reduce/resolve any problems the individual may have had. The correlations between expectancy and perceived barriers to using an EAP from both personal sources (r = -.15, p < .05) and work sources (r = -.l3, p < .05) were also significant. Based on these relationships, it appears that there is a relationship between barriers and expectancy and that as an individual perceives more barriers to using an EAP, the individual also tends to place less faith in the EAP’s ability to help resolve problems. When combining the two variables of perceived personal support/pressure and perceived personal barriers to using an EAP into a new variable identified as Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers and the two variables of perceived work support/pressure and work barriers into a new variable called Work Support/Pressure/Barriers, the correlations of these new variables with expectancy were positive and significant (both at r = .14, p < .05). Thus, this suggests that the more perceived support to using an EAP and the fewer barriers perceived, the more likely an individual was to also believe the EAP would help. Roadblocks or barriers seem to be key factors influencing exPectancy perceptions because if an individual obtains support from both work and 216 personal sources, the individual may be more likely to believe the EAP will help as long as the person perceives few hassles/barriers involved in using it. In fact, the effect of perceived hassles on EAP usage again may be influenced by irrationality. The decision making process associated with the health-related behavior (EAP usage) seems to be compromised by emotions. Going to a doctor (or counselor) often includes a little fear for many individuals. I can understand how perceptions of hassles (along with a belief in chance controlling one’s health) could encourage employees to not perceive EAP services to be of benefit and thus to not use EAP services. After all, for some individuals, no news is good news. In examining other significant correlations with expectancy, two additional variables were found to be significantly and positively related. An individual’s perception of the health values and norms of members within his/her social group was significantly related to the expectancy that an EAP will help (r = .13, p < .05). The more positive the health norms of those within one’s social group, the more the individual tended to believe the EAP would help. It could be that discussing problems and solutions with others influenced one’s belief that help was available. In addition, expectancy and perceived discrepancy were significantly related (r = .13, p < .05) which might suggest that when individuals perceived a discrepancy between their current and desired health they may also have tended to believe the EAP could resolve this. The only significant correlation between expectancy that the EAP will help and the Health Locus of Control subscales, was the correlation with the Chance HLOC subscale (r = ~.18, p < .01). Based on the relationship between expectancy and Chance H LOC and between expectancy and the work and personal barriers variables, it appears that if a person believes his/her health is a function of chance and also perceives barriers 217 to using an EAP, the person is less likely to believe the EAP will help. In addition, as discussed above, the more positive the perceptions of the health norms and behaviors of a person’s social group, along with the more encouragement received to go to an EAP from both a person’s social and work group, the more likely the person was to believe the EAP would help. So, why wasn’t a relationship found between expectancy and EAP utilization? Perhaps the lack of a relationship may be due to the low test—retest reliability of some of the items, as determined by the third pilot test (reliability of the items in the expectancy measure ranged from .51 to .75). Hypotheses #6 and #7: Effect of Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers and Work Support/Pressure/Barriers on EAP Utilization The final two hypotheses predicted a significant relationship between EAP utilization and the two support/pressure/barriers variables. The first variable was calculated by combining the personal support/pressure and personal barriers variables into Personal Support/Pressure/ Barriers to examine the overall influence of support and barrier factors in one’s personal life on EAP usage. The second variable was created by combining the work support/pressure and work barriers variables into Work Support/Pressure/ Barriers to examine the overall influence of support and barrier factors perceived in one’s work situation. Results of the two regression analyses found both variables to be significantly related to EAP utilization. This coincides with other researchers who have concluded that social pressure and attitudes are a key factor influencing behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Anderson & Bartkus, 1973; Gottlieb & Green, 1984; Hockbaum, 1958; Shephard, I985). 218 For example, Hockbaum (1958) found that social support was a decisive factor leading to participation in a preventive tuberculosis screening. Hockbaum found that individuals who voluntarily participated knew others who had also participated, while those not participating knew fewer people who intended to participate or recalled unfavorable discussions about the screening. Similarly, Shephard (1985) studied employees participating and not participating in the General Foods Corporation fitness program and found that participants had more social support from spouses and friends than nonparticipants. Gottlieb and Green (1984) found that social support was positively related to five positive health practices: smoking, exercise, alcohol use, weight maintenance, and sleep. 1 The present research indicates that perceptions of support and/or pressure appear to have a great impact on the utilization of EAP services. Given the fact that there was little relationship between desired health goal level and EAP utilization (r = —.01, n.s.) or between perceived discrepancy in health and EAP utilization (r = —.02, n.s.), individuals in the sample who sought assistance at an EAP may have done so, not for health enhancement purposes, but due to some overarching non-health reason. In addition to the regression analyses, further examination was undertaken on the relationships between the Personal and Work Support/Pressure/Barriers variables and the other major variables in the model. For example, the Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers variable was #significantly and positively correlated to the following four variables: Internal HLOC (r = .12, p < .05), perceived current health problems (r = .14, p < .05), expectancy that an EAP will help (r = .14, p < .05), and going to an EAP (r = .40, p < .01). In other words, a person tended to perceive more SUpport and fewer barriers to using an EAP when the person also had an Internal 219 HLOC, perceived more current health problems, and believed the EAP would help. In addition, the more a person perceived support and few barriers to using an EAP, the more likely the person was to use an EAP. Significant correlations were also found between Work Support/Pressure/Barriers and other key variables of interest. These are: Internal HLOC (r = .15, p < .05), Powerful Others HLOC (r = -.12, p < .05), Chance HLOC (r = -.13, p < .05), current problems (r = .26, p < .01), social group health values/norms (r = .21, p < .01), perceived discrepancy between current and desired health (r = -.16, p < .01), expectancy that an EAP will help (r = .15, p < .05), and EAP utilization (r = .24, p < .01). With regard to the Health Locus of Control variables (Internal, Powerful Others, and Chance), their relationships with Work Support/Pressure/Barriers might indicate that an Internal HLOC is related to an individual perceiving a higher level of Work Support/Pressure/Barriers (i.e., higher support and fewer barriers) while having an External HLOC (Powerful Others or Chance) is related to an individual perceiving a lower level of Work Support/Pressure/Barriers (i.e., less support and more barriers). The Significant relationships outlined above indicate that individuals who perceive a higher level of Work Support/Pressure/Barriers also tend to perceive more current health problems, more positive health values/norms of members within their social group, a more positive belief that an EAP will help, and have a greater likelihood of going to an EAP. The positive relationship between Work Support/Pressure/Barriers and perceived level of current problems might suggest that a positive work environment could potentially make it easier for individuals to recognize or identify health problems. The 220 fact that this relationship was higher between current problems and Work Support/Pressure/Barriers (r = .26, p < .01) than with Personal Support/Pressure/Barriers (r = .14, p < .05) might suggest that an individual is under different types of pressures when sharing problems in a work environment versus a personal environment. A supportive, barrier-free (regarding EAP usage) environment might be one of the key elements toward improving EAP utilization. As discussed earlier, it seems to be the employee’s perception of the absence of barriers (hassles) from both personal and work environments that could significantly influence EAP utilization. It has also been suggested that support/pressure from work sources and personal sources are separate constructs even though most research has incorporated them into one global support construct (Broadhead et al., 1983; Bruhn & Philips, 1987; Mayer, 1988). Social support from personal sources has been found to influence participation in health activities (Hammitt, 1984; Merriman, 1984; Snyder & Spreitzer, 1973). While many have suggested that organizational factors such as management support are likely to affect occupational health outcomes (Dickman & Emener, 1982; Glasgow & Terborg, 1988; Greenwood, 1983; Syme, 1986), Mayer (1988) has noted there has been little examination of management support in health research and ”no empirical evidence that management support has a significant effect on health promotion efforts at the work site." However, there is available research on the importance of supervisors in the effectiveness of alcohol—related programs (e.g., Trice & Roman, 1972) so that, although individual decision making is often compromised by emotional factors, the role of the organization and of management cannot be overlooked and additional research is needed to examine their influence. 221 In examining just the personal support/pressure and work support/pressure variables (without barriers) and their relationship with EAP utilization in the present research, personal support/pressure was significantly correlated with utilization (r = -.23, p < .05) while work support/pressure was not (r = .07, n.s.). As Mayer has suggested, these may in fact be Separate constructs. Contrary to prior research (Antonovsky & Kats, 1970; Blackwell, 1979; Champion, 1988), however, personal sources of support/pressure did not have a positive influence on health behavior (EAP usage) but rather these sources were negatively related to utilization. As discussed above, the personal sources of support/ pressure measure was negatively related to EAP utilization (1’ = -.23, p < .01) as was the personal barriers measure (r = -.46, p < .01). Finally, the negative correlation between Chance HLOC and Work Support/Pressure/Barriers is consistent with previous discussions anti seems to suggest that since Chance HLOC is less influenced by the organization, a greater challenge to organizations exists in attempting to understand other factors they may be able to influence that encourage EAP utilization behavior. Limitations and Future Research Previous EAP research has examined primarily demographic variables of EAP users to gain an understanding of what a typical profile of an EAP user resembles. Most of the data gathered to date have focused on factors that can be obtained directly from EAP records—gender, race, age, job status, income, etc. These factors offer little understanding of why some employees utilize EAP services while others do not. Furthermore, many of the conclusions reached on EAP participation factors have often been inconclusive or contradictory. 222 The present research has contributed to prior research on EAP utilization in the following ways. The research is based on the premise that EAP usage is a function of a decision that is made by an individual to utilize EAP services. Simply studying the means and variances of various demographic variables of EAP users, as conducted by past researchers, does not aid in understanding this decision process. It is necessary to better understand this decision process by examining factors which might influence the decision. To aid in this understanding, the present research incorporated the following. First, individual personality variables predicted to influence health attitudes and behaviors were examined. Second, beliefs held by individuals which were hypothesized to be critical in making a decision regarding EAP utilization were analyzed-in particular, beliefs examining expectations regarding the EAP’s ability to improve one’s health, beliefs about the health consciousness of one’s social group, and beliefs about one’s health-related goals were explored. Third, a control theory framework was developed to provide a structure to studying the EAP decision process. The potential influences were incorporated into a control theory model which was based on the concept of a standard or goal being used as a reference value in a process where sensed information about one’s current state is compared to this standard or goal. If a discrepancy between the goal and current state existed, it was predicted that a force is created that motivates an individual to reduce the discrepancy (e.g., to seek EAP assistance). The present research also went beyond past EAP utilization research by not only including EAP users in the sample (as past research has done) but also individuals who had not used EAP services despite the fact that these non-EAP users may have had problems that EAP’s could provide assistance on. Thus, the dependent variable was 223 expanded to include EAP usage and non-usage so a better understanding of factors influencing this decision might be obtained. In addition, a positive factor in measuring the dependent variable, EAP usage, is that this behavior was measured in two ways--via a self-report measure as well as via EAP records--so verification of usage/non-usage was able to be undertaken. While the above presents some contributions to EAP utilization research, there are some limitations to the present research which should be considered when examining the results of this study. First, the research was conducted with employees who were all members of one of five unions included in the sample. Thus, the generalizability of the results to other employees who are non-union may be questionable. However, while the sample selected may have been limited to the unions included in the research, on the positive side is the fact that the unions represented positions that included clerical, maintenance, technical, and supervisory occupations. In addition, the overall return rate was excellent at 47%, which was well represented by both EAP users (with a 59% return rate) and non‘EAP users (with a 42% return rate). A second limitation to the research may be the setting under which the present study was undertaken. All employees included in the sample had equal access to an on- site (internal) EAP which had offices lo'cated on the premises of the organization. The extent the results are generalizable to external or contracted-out EAPs is uncertain. A third potential limitation is that the model in Figure 9 cannot be fully tested (from left to right) using causal modeling techniques. Because the model developed is a flow model rather than a causal model, it was not possible to directly examine relationships among all the control theory components (i.e., health goals, feedback on health status, discrepancy) and EAP utilization. While some of the components within 224 the model might be useful conceptually in guiding future research on EAP utilization, at the present time the relative usefulness of utilizing a control theory framework to examine EAP usage behavior is questionable. Because human behavior tends to be goal directed, the present study predicted that a perceived discrepancy between an individual’s desired health and current health state would motivate the person to reduce this discrepancy. The goal that was examined as the basis for motivating an individual to improve his/her health was a person’s desired health goal level. However, the present study found no relationship (at least correlationally) between desired health goal level and EAP utilization so the idea that an individual’s health goal directs his/her behavior may not be the apprOpriate goal to examine. Also, as previously discussed, the present research also indicated that it may be difficult to obtain variance on the desired health goal measure. Thus, it is unlikely that a control theory model of EAP utilization that is guided by a discrepancy between a person’s desired and current health state will contribute much to understanding this decision process. Since control theory is based on the concept of behavior being goal-directed, the use of this theory may still be relevant in examining health-seeking behaviors if we can determine the proper goal that is driving this behavior. Future research might focus on trying to understand the specific factors or goals involved; that is, exactly what goal the individual is attempting to attain when seeking EAP assistance--the one to be healthy or the one to avoid a negative consequence? The present research suggests some interesting findings regarding the variables examined. For example, much of the past research has indicated that influences from others regarding their attitudes, beliefs, norms, etc. have a significant effect on an individual’s own beliefs, attitudes and behaviors (e.g., Haskell & Blair, 1980; Loy, 225 McPherson & Kenyon, 1978; wynne, 1986). However, the present research suggests that social influences alone may have little influence on health goals and on a specific health behavior-EAP utilization. This may be because individual characteristics/personality traits may have more of an influence on usage than has previously been examined. For example, the control issue seems to be important as it relates to EAP utilization. An individual’s decision to seek help seems to be influenced by his/her perception of who primarily controls one’s health (internal forces or external forces of chance/luck) as well as personal and work-related factors. The issue of control arises not only with regard to control over one’s health (i.e., Health Locus of Control) but also control over the situation. For example, while the belief that luck or chance controls one’s health can influence non-EAP utilization behavior, is there a point where the support/pressure from one’s boss or spouse encourages (forces) the employee to seek EAP services despite this belief in “chance” (e.g., the threat of losing one’s job or the threat of divorce)? Also, in addition to the Health Locus of Control subscales, there may be other individual characteristics that significantly affect EAP usage which might be examined in the future (e.g., coping style, degree of self-focus). The issue of influence from others requires future study in several other areas. In the present research, the social group health values and norms measure was not Significantly related to health goals. However, this does not necessarily imply these factors don’t influence other health—related beliefs or behaviors. The social group health values/norms measure was found to be significantly correlated with (1) believing others control one’s health (i.e., Powerful Others HLOC), (2) the individual’s perception of his/her current health status, and (3) the belief that an EAP will be helpful. Therefore, social influences may still be important in influencing other attitudes and behaviors. 226 Since one of the organization’s goals is to encourage EAP usage among "troubled" employees, further research examining how employees are potentially influenced by others in the health area should be examined. Summary Organizations have recognized that troubled employees cost the organizations more in the long—run than if the employees’ problems were diagnosed and treated early. Employees who have personal, social, emotional, and other problems are less productive and have higher illness, absenteeism, accident, and disability rates. Therefore, it is in the organizations’ own best interest to understand factors that encourage troubled employees to seek or not seek assistance. The assistance examined in the present research focused on Employee Assistance Programs. The purpose of the present research was twofold: to develop a control theory framework that might be useful in guiding future EAP utilization research and to test this conceptual framework. The model incorporated variables that have previously not been examined in EAP utilization research. Health history and social group health values and norms were found to have no significant influence on health goals. While not all hypotheses were fully supported, in general the perception of who (self or others) or what (chance or luck) controls one’s health seems to be an important variable influencing both health goals and EAP utilization. In addition, the combined effects of Support/pressure and barriers had a significant impact on EAP usage. However, the expectancy or belief that an EAP can help reduce/resolve health problems was found to have no significant impact on seeking EAP assistance. Explanations as to why these results may have occurred were offered. 227 An individual’s perception of control, support, pressure and barriers, like anyone’s perception, appears to be a complicated phenomenon. No single variable is robust enough to predict EAP participation alone. Cultural, social, psychological and other variables all interact to influence health behavior. EAP utilization is a complicated decision-making process and organizations face a challenge when attempting to determine how they might influence this behavior. By examining a combination of variables, we may significantly increase the ability to explain participation factors. Future research is necessary to examine the separate and combined effects of Health Locus of Control and social influences from both personal and work environments on EAP utilization. LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Abdellah, F ., & Levine, E. (1979). Better patient care through nursing research (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company. Adams, S. L. (1959). Health misconceptions among students enrolled in freshmen health classes at the University of Oregon. (Master’s thesis, University of Oregon.) Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. Akabas, S. H., Kurzman, P., & Kolben, N. (1979). Labor and industrial settings: Sites for social work practice. New York: Industrial Social Welfare Center, CUSSW. Albert, W. C., Smythe, P. C., & Brook, R. C. (1985). Promises to keep: An evaluator’s perspective on employee assistance programs. Evaluation and Pro am Plannin , _8_, 175-182. Albrecht, S. L., & Goldman, P. (1985). Men, women, and informal organization in manufacturing. Sogiological Focus 18 279-288. )—9 Alderman, M. (1984). Worksite treatment of hypertension. In J. D. Matarazzo, S. M. Weiss, J. A. Herd, N. E. Miller, & S. M. Weiss (Eds), Behavioral health: A handbook of health enhancement and disease prevention (pp. 862-869). New York: Wiley. Alheim, W. R. (1973). An analysis of the effectiveness of traditional teaching, team teaching, and contract teaching methods of basic health education to junior college students. (Doctoral dissertation, The Florida State University.) Allen, J. S., & Allen, R. F. (1986). Achieving health promotion objectives through cultural change systems. American Journal of Health Promotion, 1, 42-29. Anderson, J. G., & Bartkus, D. E. (1973). Choice of medical care: A behavioral model of health and illness behavior. Journal of Health & Social Behavior 14 348-362. ’—) Antonovsky, A., & Kats, R. (1970). The model dental patient--An empirical study of preventive health behavior. Social Science and Medicine, 4, 367-380. 228 229 Applebaum, S. (1982). A human resource counseling model: The alcoholic employee. Personnel Administrator, 21(8), 35-44. Archer, R. L., Hormuth, S. E., & Berg, J. H. (1979). Self-disclosure under conditions of self-awareness. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York. Asma, F., Eggert, R., & Hilker, R. (1971). Long-term experience with rehabilitation of alcoholic employees. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 12, 581—585. Attitudes toward co-Operation in a health examination survey. Public Health Service Series D, No.6, US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare: Health Statistics. Babbage, C. (1835). On the economyof machinery and manufacturers (4th ed.). London: Charles Knight. Balch, P., & Ross, A. W. (1975). Predicting success in weight reduction as a function of locus of control: A unidimensional and multidimensional approach. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 119. Bandura, A. (1971). Social learnipg theory. New York: General Learning Press. Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American limholtngist, 3_7_, 122-147. Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluation and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems. Journal of Personalit and Social Psychology 45 1027-1028. ,—7 Bandura, A., & Walters, R. (1963). Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, Reinhart, and Winston. Barker, J. W. (1987). Perceived wellness needs of university employees and variations in relation to personal and employment characteristics (Doctoral Dissertation, North Carolina State University). Diss rt tion Abstracts Intern tional, fl(02),SECA, 204. Baughman, M. K. (1978). The relationship of locus of control and value beliefs to health status and behavior among clerical workers. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati, 1978). Dissertation Abstracts International, 3, 5217A-5218A. Baxter, A. K. (1976, Oct—Nov). University employees as people: A counseling service for faculty and staff. College and University Personnel Assoeiation Journal, 2i, 44- 49. 230 Baxter, J. (1978). Employee assistance programs in historical perspective: The melding of economics and- humanlconcerns. Conference Proceedings, August 7-9, 1977, Employee Assistance Programs in Higher Education. Columbia: University of Missouri and NIAAA. Baxter, J. (1981, March/April). The dollars and sense of EAPs. Alcoholism Magazine, pp. 29-30. Beer, M. (1980). Organization change and development. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Bellack, H. S., Rozensky, R., & Schwartz, J. (1974). A comparison of two forms of self- monitoring--a behavioral weight reduction program. Behavior Therapy, 5(4), 523- 530. Belloc, N. B. (1973). Relationship of health practices and mortality. Preventive Medicine, 2, 67-81. Belloc, N. B., & Breslow, L. (1972). Relationship of physical health status and health practices. Preventive Medicine, 1, 409-421. Bellows, R. (1961). Psychology offipersonnel in business and industry (3rd ed). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice—Hall, Inc. Berry, R. E., & Boland, J. P. (1977). The work-related costs of alcohol abuse. In C. J. Schramm (Ed.), Alcoholism and its treatment in industry (pp. 37-48). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Besch, C., Boyle, G., Gott, S., Hutchinson, K., et al. (1977). A consumer look at EAP’s: University of Missouri—Columbia personnel, faculty, union, state OPC. Conference Proceedings, August 1-3, 1976, Employee Assistance Programs in Higher Education. Columbia: University of Missouri and NIAAA. Best, J. A. (1975). Tailoring smoking withdrawal procedures to personality and motivational differences. Journal of Consultin and Clinical Ps cholo ,4;(1), 1-8. Beyer, J., & Trice, H. (1978). Implementing change: Alcoholism policies in work organizations. New York: MacMillan. Bierman, M. E. (1981). Characteristics of selected Wisconsin EAP’s. In EAP Digest Annual 1981-1982, Troy, MI: Performance Resource Press. Birkland, S. P. (1983). Staffing employee assistance programs. EAP Di est, 3(4), 24-27. Blackwell, B. (1979). Treatment adherence: A contemporary overview. W, 20, 27-35. Blau, P. M., & Scott, R. W. (1962). Formal or anization: A com )arative a roach. San Francisco: Chandler. 231 Bloom, S. (1986). Employee assistance programs: The structure of counseling applications withinorganizational settings. (Doctoral dissertation, Southern Illinois University at Carbondale). Borsky, P. N., & Sagan, O. K. (1959). Motivations toward health examinations. American Journal of Public Health, _4_9_, 514-527. Bowler, E. M. & Dawson, F. T. (1948). Counseliagyemfiployees. New York: Prentice— Hall, Inc. Brandes, S. D. (1976). American welfare capitalism 1880-1940. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Brewer, L. 8., & McAvoy, K. (1986). St. Mary’s bridges the EAP stigma gap. E P Digest, _6_, 80. Breznitz, S., & Goldberger, L. (1982). Stress research at a crossroads. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds), Handbook of stress (pp. 1-6). New York: Free Press. Brim, O. G., Jr. (1968). Adult socialization. In J. A. Clausen (Ed.), Socialization and society. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co. Brink, S. D. (1983). Deflating health care costs. Business Insurance, 11(52), 17-18. Broadhead, W. E., Kaplan, B. H., James, S. A., Wagner, E. H., Schoenbach, V. J., Grimson, R., Heyden, S., Tibblin, G., & Gehlbach, S. H. (1983). The epidemiologic evidence for a relationship between social support and health. WW Epidemiology, 1g, $21-$37. Brockner, J. (1979). The effects of self-esteem, success-failure, and self—consciousness on task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1732-1741. Brodsky, C. M. (1977). Long term work stress in teachers and prison guards. Journal of Oceupational Medicine, 19, 133. Brown, B. S. (1973). Forward. In Hyman J. Weiner, Sheila Akabas, & John J. Sommer (Eds), Mental health care in the world of work. New York: Association Press. Brown, R. C. (1983). Industrial Chaplaincy in America--a profile. Research paper for the National Institute of Business and Industrial Chaplains, Ridgecrest, NC, 1981. Winston-Salem, NC: R. J. Reynolds Industries. Bruhn, J. G., & Cordova, F. D. (1977). A developmental approach to learning wellness behavior part I: Infancy to early adolescence. Health Values: Achieving High-Level Wellness, 1(16), 246-254. Bruhn, J. G., Cordova, F. D., Williams, J. A., & Fuentes, R. G. (1977). The wellness process. Journal of Community Health, 2, 209—221. 232 Bruhn, J. G., & Philips, B. U. (1987). A developmental basis for social support. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,_1_Q, 213-229. Burawoy, M. (1979). Manufacturing consent: Changes in the labor process under capitalism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bureau of National Affairs (BNA). (1974). Personnel policies, (No.. 333), Washington, D. C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, _2__4_5_: 151-1452154. Bureau of National Affairs (BNA). (1983). Controlling health care costs: Crisis in employee benefits. Washington, DC: Author. Bureau of National Affairs (BNA). (1987). Employee assistance programs: benefits, problems, and prospects (BSP 59). Washington, DC: The Bureau of National Affairs, Inc. Bureau of the Census. (1986). Statistical abstract of the United States: 1986. Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office. Busch, E. J. (1981). Developing an employee assistance program. Personnel Journal, 60, 708-71 1. Cahill, M. H. (1983). Tailoring EAP services to organizational needs. EAP Di est, 3(4), 32-35. Campion, M. A., & Lord, R. G. (1982). A control systems conceptualization of the goal- setting and changing process. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 30, 265-287. Caplan, R. (1976). Adhering to medical regimens: Pilot experiments inypatient education and social support. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research. Caplan, R D., Cobb, 8., French, J. R. P., Harrison, R. V., & Pinneau, S. R. (1975). Job u— demands and worker health. U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, HEW Publication No. (NIOSH) 75-160. Caplan, R. D., Robison, S. A. R., French, J. R. P., Caldwell, J. R., & Shinn, M. (1976). Adhering to medical regimen. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan. Carr, J. L., & Hellan, R. T. (1980). Improving corporate performance through employee assistance programs. Business Horizons, 2_3(2), 57-60. Carver, C. S. (1974). Facilitation of physical aggression through objective self-awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, m, 365-370. 233 Carver, C. S. (1975). Physical aggression as a function of objective self-awareness and . . attitudes towardpunishment- Journalof Experimental Social Psychology, _1_1,, 510- 519. Carver, C. S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1251-1281. Carver, C. S., Blaney, P. H., & Scheier, M. F. (1979a). Focus of attention, chronic expectancy, and responses to a feared stimulus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1186-1195. Carver, C. S., Blaney, P. H., & Scheier, M. F. (197%). Reassertion and giving up: The interactive role of self-directed attention and outcome expectancy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3_7_, 1859-1870. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, fl, 111—135. Cascio, W. F. (1986). Managing human resources: Productivi_ty.10ruality of work life, m. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Cassel, J. (1976). The contribution of social environment to host resistance. American Journal of Epidemiology, 113(2), 107-23. Castro, J. (1986, March 17). Battling the enemy within: Companies fight to drive illegal drugs out of the workplace. Time, pp. 52-61. Champion, P. S. (1988). A comparison of telephone and face-to-face intervention within an employee assistance program (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Berkeley). Christiansen, K. E. (1981). The determinants of health promoting behavior (Doctoral dissertation, College of Nursing, Rush University). Clausen, J. A. (1968a). Socialization as a concept and as a field of study. In J. A. Clausen (Ed), Socialization and society. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co. Clausen, J. A. (1968b). Perspectives on childhood socialization. In J. A. Clausen (Ed.), Socialization and society. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co. Coan, R. W. (1973). Personality variables associated with cigarette smoking. Journal of Personali and Social Ps holo , _2_(_)(1), 86-204. 234 Coburn, D., & Pope, C. (1974). Socioeconomic status and preventive health behavior. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 1_5__, 67-78. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, S., & Hoberman, H. (1983). Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, _13, 99-125. Cohen, 8., & Syme, S. L. (Eds). (1985). Social support and health. New York: Academic. Coleman, T. L., Jr. (1984). A study of the nature, content, and process of employee assistance programs at selected higher education institutions (Doctoral Dissertation, North Texas State University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 43(03), SECA, 759. Compcare. (1981). Employee assistance program: A dollar sense issue. Newport Beach, CA: Comprehensive Care Corporation. C00per, C. L. (1981). The stress check: Cogingwith the stresses of life and work. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. County of Alameda, California, Division of Occupational Health Services Personnel Department. (1978, May 30). Evaluative study of the Alameda County EAP and occupational health services, Inc.. Alameda, CA: Author. Crockett, W. (1977). Introducing change to a government agency. In P. Mirvis & D. Berg (Eds), Failures in organizational development: Cases and essays for learning (pp. 201-219). New York: Wiley—Interscience. Cromidas, C. A. (1987). Some aspects of compliance in an employee assistance program (Master’s Thesis, University of Texas at Arlington). Masters Abstracts International, 26(04), 386. Crookston, B. B. (1975). Human development: Actualizing people in actualizing organizations. ,loarnal of College Student Personnel, 16(5), 368—375. DeFuentes, N. (1986). A study of implementation and evaluation of an aerospace employee assistance program (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Los Angeles). Dissertation Abstracts International, _4_7(05), SECA, 2209. Delaney, T. (1983, October). Executive director’s comment. The ALMACAN,_13(10), 2. DHHS (Department of Health and Human Services). (1982, March). Re ort to the U.S. Con ress on fed ral activities on alcohol abuse and alcoholism FY 1980. Rockville, MD: NIAAA. 235 Dickman, F., & Emener, W. G. (1982). Employee assistance programs: Basic concepts, . attributes, and an evaluation. Personnel Administrator, 2_2, 55-62. DiMarco, C. A. (1985). An examination of the existing relationships among specific health behaviors, health locus of control, stress manifestations, extra-curricular activities and selected demographics within a university population (Doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University). Drucker, P. E. (1974). Management tasks, responsibilities and practices. New York: Harper and Row. Dubos, R. J. (1965). Man adapting. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Dunkin, W. (1981, March/April). The EAP movement, past and present. Alcoholism Magazine, pp. 27-28. Dunn, D. A. (1980). The relationship of health locus of control and health value to medical self-care knowledge and attitudes of undergraduate and graduate health education majors in Oregon (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon). DuPont, R. (1979, January 17-19). The control of alcoholism and drugabuse in industry. Background paper written for the National Conference on Health Promotional Programs in Occupational Settings. Washington, DC: Department of Health, Education and Welfare. Duval, S. 1., Wicklund, R. A., & Fine, R. L. (1972). Avoidance of objective self- awareness under conditions of high and low intra-self discrepancy. In S. Duval & R. A. Wicklund (Eds), A theorypf opjective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press. Eaton, W. W. (1978). Life events, social support, and psychiatric symptoms: A re- analysis of the New Haven data. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19(2), 230-4. Egdahl, R. H., & Walsh, D. C. (1980). Mental wellness programs for employees. New York: Springer-Verlag, Inc. E. mployee assistance program services for faculty and staff employees of Michigan State University (1990). Annual Report July 1, 1989 - June 30, 1990. East Lansing, MI: Employee Assistance Program. Employee health-care costs rose 7.9% in 1987. (1988, Feb. 10). Lo An eles Times, sec. 4, 17. Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Seience, 126, 129-136. Erez, M. (1977). Feedback: A necessary condition for the goal-setting-performance relationship. ournal of A )Ii d Ps holo , 6_2_, 624-627. 236 Erfurt, J. C., & Foote, A. (1977). Occupational employee assistance programs for substance abuse andmental health problems. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan--Wayne State University. Everly, G. S. (1984). The develOpment of occupational stress management programs. In G. S. Everly & R. H. G. Feldman (Eds), Occupational health promotion (pp. 49- 73). New York: Wiley. Everly, G. S., & Feldman, R. H. G. (Eds). (1984). Occupational health promotion. New York: Wiley. Fabrega, H. (1974). Disease and social behavior: An interdisciplinaryperspective. Boston: MIT Press. F abricatore, J. M., & Rogal, R. A. (1984). Employee assistance programs: Psychological, organizational and professional considerations. Report of the task force on EAPs. Los Angeles, CA: California State Psychological Assn. Farber, L. R. (1982). Giving employees in crisis what they need. Supervisory Management, 11(1), 34-39. Feather, N. T. (1966). Effects of prior success and failure on expectations of success and subsequent performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 287-298. Feather, N. T. (1967). Level of aspiration and performance variability. Journal of Personality and Social Pchhology, p, 37-46. Feather, N. T., & Saville, M. R. (1967). Effects of amount of prior success and failure on expectations of success and subsequent task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, _5_, 226-232. Featherston, H. J., & Bednarek, R. J. (1981). A positive demonstration of concern for employees. Personnel Administrator, 23(9), 43—47. Felix, M. R., Stunkard, M. D., Cohen, R. Y., & Cooley, N. (1985). Health promotion at the worksite. Preventive Medicine 14 99-108. 7—, Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. E., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self- consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consultin and Clini al Psychology, 43, 522-527. Fever, B. (1983). A peer referral model that works. EAP Di est, 3(4), 36-37. Fielding, J. E. (1984a). Corporate health management. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Fielding, J. (1984b). Health promotion and disease prevention at the worksite. Anpug] Review P Ii He lth, 3, 237-65. 237 Fisch, M. (1974). Internal versus external ego orientation and family planning effectiveness among poorblackwomen. . Dissertation Abstracts International, 33(2- B), 1045-1046. F ishbein, M. (1966). The relationships between beliefs, attitudes and behavior. In S. Feldman, (Ed.), Cognitive consistency: Motivational antecedents and behavioral consequents (pp. 199-223). New York: Academic Press. Fisher, G. L. (1983). Employee counseling programs and related services in the public sector. In J. S. Manuso (Ed.) Oceupational clinical psychology. New York: Praeger Publishers. Fly, R. D. (1980). Why rotating shifts sharply reduce productivity. Supervisofiry Management, 23(1), 16—21. Follman, J. F. (1978). Helping the troubled employee. New York: Amacon. Foote, A., Erfurt, J., Straugh, P., and Guzzardo, T. (1978). Cost effectiveness of occupational EAPs. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Institute of Labor and Industrial Relations. Ford, R. C., & McLaughlin, F. S. (1981). Employee assistance programs: A descriptive survey of ASPA members. Personnel Administrator, 2_6,(9), 29-35. Forrest, D. V. (1983). Employee assistance programs in the 1980’s: Expanding career options for counselors. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 32(2), 105-107. Frank, L. L., & Hackman, J. R. (1975). A failure of job enrichment: The case of the change that wasn’t. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 1_1, 413-436. French, J. R. P., Caplan, R. D., & Van Harrison, R. (1982). The mechanisms of job stress and strain. New York: Wiley. French, W. L., & Bell, C. H. (1984). Organization development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Gam, J., Sauser, W. 1., Evans, K. L., & Lair, C. V. (1983). Implementing an employee assistance program. Journal of Employment Counseling, 20(2), 61—69. Ganster, D. C., Mayes, B. T., Sime, W. E., & Tharp, G. D. (1982). Managing organizational stress: A field experiment. Journal of A lied Ps cholo , 6_7, 533- 542. Gelso, C. J., and Johnson, H. D. (1983). Explorations in time-limited counseling and psychotherapy. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University. General Mills American Family Forum. (1980, May 13). Private sector initiatives to romote ment l h lth. Washington, DC: Author. 238 Gibbons, F. X. (1978). Sexual standards and reactions to pornography: Enhancing behavioral consistency through self-focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36 976-987. ’—-—, Glasgow, R., & Terborg, J. R. (1988). Occupational health promotion programs to reduce cardiovascular risk. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 365- 373. Glasser, M. A. (1969, November). Prepaid psychiatric care experience with UAW members. American Journal of Psychiatry. Goldman, P. (1983). The labor process and the sociology of organization. Perspectives in organizational sociology; Theory and researeh, 2, 49-81. Goodstadt, M. 8., Simpson, R. I., & Loranger, P. D. (1987). Health promotion: A conceptual integration. American Journal of Health Promotion 1 58-63. 2—7 Googins, B. (1975). Employee assistance programs. Social Work, 23(6), 464-467. Googins, B. (1984). EAP: An ecological perspective. In H. Grimes (Ed.), EAP Research. Troy, MI: Performance Resource Press, Inc. Googins, B. (1987). Occupational social work. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Googins, B., & Kurtz, N. (1980). Factors inhibiting supervisory referrals to occupational alcoholism intervention programs. Journal_of Studies on Alcohol, 4_Q, 1196—1208. Googins, B. & Kurtz, N. (1981). Discriminating participating and nonparticipating supervisors in occupational alcoholism programs. Journal of Dru 'Issues,2, 199-216. Gordon, G. A. (1973). Occupational programming. Michi an Challen *e, HQ), 27-28. Goslin, D. A. (Ed.) (1969). Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 1-19). Chicago: Rand McNaIIy. Gottlieb, B. (1982). Social support in the workplace. In D. Biegel and A. Naperstek (Eds), Community support systems and mental health. New York: Springer. Gottlieb, N. H., & Green, L. W. (1984). Life events, social network, life-style, and health: An analysis of the 1979 national survey of personal health practices and consequences. Health Education Quarterly, fl“), 91-105. Gould, G., & McKenzie, C. (1984). The expanding scope of industrial social work. In E. McBroom (Ed.), Social Work Papers, Vol. 18. Los Angeles, CA: School of Social Work, University of Southern California. Gouldner, A. W. (1954). Patterns of industrial bureaucracy. New York: Free Press. 239 Graham, R. S. (1984). The corporate medical department: History of the development -of, occupational healthprogramsandan overviewof. the future. .InrJ. Manusco (Ed.), Occupational Clinical Psychology. New York: Praeger Publishers. Gramse, C. A. (1982). The relationship of internal-external health expectencies, value of health, health beliefs and health behavior regarding breast self-exam in women (Doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1982). Dissertation Almtracts International 43 385-8. ,—-, Green, L. (1970). Status identity and preventive health behavior. Pacific Health Education Reports, 1, 1-18. Greenwood, M. (1983). Communicating EAPs. EAP Digest, 3(4), 30-31. Grimes, C. H. (1980). A needs assessment for a faculty/employee assistance program in the Florida State University system (Doctoral dissertation, Graduate School of the University of Florida). Dissertation Abstracts International, 4_2_(02), SECA, 568. Hall, D. (1983, June 20). Helping employees treat problems of drugs and alcohol. New England Business, 3(11), 52-53. Hammitt, S. A. (1984). The influence of social group, past experience, and attitude on participation and nonparticipation in sports and physical activities (Doctoral dissertation, University of Tennessee). Dissertation Abstracts International, 4_5(02), SECA, 452. Harvey, A. (1976). Risky and safe contraceptors: Some personality factors. Journal of Psychology, _92, 109-112. Haskell, W. L., & Blair, S. N. (1980). The physical activity component of health promotion in occupational settings. WW, 9_5, 109-118. Hayghe, H. (1984). Research summaries. Monthly Labor Review, 31-33. Herring, R. A. (1987). Change in work related attitudes among alcoholics in an employee assistance program: An empirical investigation (Doctoral dissertation, The Florida State University). Hirsch, B. J. (1980). Natural support systems and coping with major life changes. American Journal of Community Psychology, 8(2), 159-72. Hobson, G. W. (1982). Utilizing the systems approach to develop, implement, and evaluate an employee assistance program (Doctoral dissertation, Union for Experimenting Colleges and Universities). Dissertation Abstracts International, fl, 2029. 240 Hochbaum, G. M. (1958). Public participation in medical screening programs: A sociological study. -. Public HealthService, PHS Pub. #572, U.S. Government Printing Office. Hoffman, E. & Roman, P. (1984). Effects of supervisory style and experientially based frames of reference on organizational alcoholism programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 4_5_(3), 260-267. Holden, C. (1973, January). Alcoholism: On the job referrals mean early detection, treatment. Science, 179, 363-365. Hollenbeck, J. R. (1989). Control theory and the perception of work environments: The effects of focus of attention on affective and behavioral reactions to work. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 406-430. Hollenbeck, J. R., & Brief A. P. (1987). Self-regulation in the workplace: Towards a unified approach to understanding worker attitudes and behaviors. In R. Schuler (Ed.), Readings in Personnel and Human Resource Management. St. Paul, MN: West Publishing. Hollenbeck, J. R., & Brief A. P. (1987). The effects of individual differences and goal origin on goal setting and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decisien Processes 40 392-414. 7—-—) Hollenbeck, J. R., Ilgen, D. R., Crampton, S. M. (1990). Lower back disability in occupational settings: A focus on the management of prevention, treatment, and recovery. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, Department of Management, East Lansing, MI. Hollenbeck, J. R., & Williams, C. R. (1987). Goal importance, self-focus, and the goal- setting process. Journal of Applied Psychology, 22, 204-211. House, J. S. (1974). Occupational stress and physical health. In J. O. O’Toole (Ed.), Work and th ualit of lif : Resource a ers for work in Americ (pp. 147-170). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Howe, C. (1983). Establishing employee recreation programs. Journal of Physical Education Recreation and Dance, _5_4, 34-52. Hoye, J. C. (1965). Pre-test and post-test of health information possessed by students enrolled in the course health information for teachers (Master’s Thesis, University of Wisconsin). Hung, K. K. (1988). Factors in employee assistance program usage (Master’s Thesis, California State University). Mas r A str t In rn ti n l,21(03), 416. 241 Ilgen, D. R. (1990). Health issues at work: Opportunities for industrial/organizational psychology. American Psychologist, 43, 273—283. Ilgen, D. R., Nebecker, D. M., & Pritchard, R. R. (1981). Expectancy theory measures: An empirical comparison in an experimental simulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 28, 189—223. Ilgen, D. R., & Swisher, S. N. (1989). An integrated approach to health in the workplace. In E. M. Welch (Ed.), Strategies for lower costs and reducing workers’ spffering (pp. 117—134). Fort Washington, PA: LRP Publications. Inkeles, A. (1968). Society structure and child socialization. In J. A. Clausen (Ed.), Socialization and society. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co. Ivancevich, J. M., & Matteson, M. T. (1980). Optimizing human resources: A case for preventive health and stress management. Organizational Dynamics, 2, 5-25. Jacobs, B. A. (1983). Sound minds, bodies . . . and savings. Industry Week, 218, 67-68. James, W., Woodruff, A., & Werner, W. (1965). Effect of internal and external control upon changes in smoking behavior. Journal of Consultingfsychology, 2_9, 184-186. Jellinek, E. M. (1960). The disease coneept of alcoholism. New Haven, CT: College University Press; in association with New Brunswick, NJ: Hill Tour Press. Jerrell, J. M., & Rightmyer, J. F. (1982). Evaluating employee assistance programs: A review of methods, outcomes, and future directions. Evaluation and Program Planning, 3, 255-267. Jeuchter, W. M., & Utne, T. (1982). Wellness: Addressing the "whole" person. Training and Development Journal, 3_6_(5), 112—116. Johnson, V. (1973). I’ll quit tomorrow. New York: Harper & Row. Johnston, R. G. (1971). A study of drug abuse among employees in Akron, Ohio. Akron, OH: Bureau of Business and Economic Research, University of Akron. Jones, D. (1983). Performance benchmarks for the comprehensive employee assistance programs. Center City, MN: Hazelden Foundation. Jorrisch, M. R. (1986). Variables affecting compliance to treatment in an employee assistance program (Doctoral dissertation, University of Louisville). Dissertation Abstracts International, 41(10), SECA, 3664. Judd, P. M. (1980). Treatment success in occupational alcoholism programs (Doctoral dissertation, Brandeis University). Dissertation Abstr ct International, 41, 2294. ngn,l). psych ngn,l). Arie asdj ngn,l] work sufis Inkeks, Sari lvancev prev Jacobs, lanes' upo ldhnel llni Ierrefl, rev Pp Ieuchn m IOIlnsc I0hnst Ill Joties, pg I0rns< 385 H Judd, d1 241 Ilgen, D. R. (1990). Health issues at work: Opportunities for industrial/organizational psychology. American Psychologist, 4_5, 273-283. Ilgen, D. R., Nebecker, D. M., & Pritchard, R. R. (1981). Expectancy theory measures: An empirical comparison in an experimental simulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 28, 189-223. Ilgen, D. R., & Swisher, S. N. (1989). An integrated approach to health in the workplace. In E. M. Welch (Ed.), Strategies for lower costs and reducing workers’ suffering (pp. 117-134). Fort Washington, PA: LRP Publications. Inkeles, A. (1968). Society structure and child socialization. In J. A. Clausen (Ed.), Socialization and society. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co. Ivancevich, J. M., & Matteson, M. T. (1980). Optimizing human resources: A case for preventive health and stress management. Organizational Dynamics, 2, 5-25. Jacobs, B. A. (1983). Sound minds, bodies . . . and savings. lndustpLWeek, 213, 67-68. James, W., Woodruff, A., & Werner, W. (1965). Effect of internal and external control upon changes in smoking behavior. Journal of Consultin Ps holo ,2_9_, 184-186. Jellinek, E. M. (1960). The disease coneept of alcoholism. New Haven, CT: College University Press; in association with New Brunswick, NJ: Hill Tour Press. Jerrell, J. M., & Rightmyer, J. F. (1982). Evaluating employee assistance programs: A review of methods, outcomes, and future directions. Evaluation and Program. Mg, 3, 255-267. Jeuchter,W. M., & Utne, T. (1982). Wellness: Addressing the "whole" person. Training and Development Journal, 33(5), 112—116. Johnson, V. (1973). I’ll quit tomorrow. New York: Harper & Row. Johnston, R. G. (1971). A stu of dru abuse amon m 10 ees in Akron Ohio. Akron, OH: Bureau of Business and Economic Research, University of Akron. Jones, D. (1983). Performance benehmarks for the comprehensive employee assistance W. Center City, MN: Hazelden Foundation. Jorrisch, M. R. (1986). Variables affecting compliance to treatment in an employee assistance program (Doctoral dissertation, University of Louisville). Dissertation WW. 511(10), SECA, 3664. Judd, P. M. (1980). Treatment success in occupational alcoholism programs (Doctoral dissertation, Brandeis University). Dissertation Abstra ts In ernational, 41, 2294. 242 Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, R. (1980). Convoys over the life course: Attachments, roles ...and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. Brim (Eds), Life-span development and pehayior (Vol. 3). Boston: Lexington Press. Kaplan, B. H., & Cassel, J. C. (1977). Social support and health. Health Education Monographs, 3(2), 129-137. Kaplan, C., & Cowles, A. (1978). Health locus of control and health value in the prediction of smoking reduction. Health Education Monographs, 3(2), 129-137. Karoly, P. (1985). The logic and character of assessment in health psychology: Perspectives and possibilities. In P. Karoly, Measurement Strategies in Health Psychology (pp. 3-45). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Kasl, S. V., & Cobb, S. (1966). Health behavior, illness behavior and sick role behavior. I. Health and illness behavior. Archives of Environmental Health 12 246-266. 7—) Katz, D., & Kahn, R. (1966). The sociology of organizations. New York: Wiley. Kegeles, S. (1969). A field experimental attempt to change beliefs and behavior of women in an urban ghetto. Health and Social Behavior. 10, 115-124. Kernan, M. C., & Lord, R. G. (1985). Multiple versus single goals: Effects of valence and feegbaek speeificity. Unpublished manuscript. Kernan, M. C., & Lord, R. G. (1987). The effects of participative versus assigned goals and feedback on goal commitment and performance in a multi-trial laboratory task. Motivation and Emotion, fl. Kilburg, R. (1980). Metaphysics and professional resistance to program evaluation. Evaluation and Program Planning, 3, 185-190. Kimball, C. P., MD. (1981). The biopsy/phosocial approach to the patient. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Kinney, J., & Leaton, G. (1983). Loosening the grip: A handbook 3f alcohol W. St. Louis: C. V. Mosby. Kirscht, J. P. (1983). Preventive health behavior: A review of research and issues. H I h Ps holo , 2, 277-302. Klein, H. J. (1989). An integrated control theory model of work motivation. Academy MW, 14., 150-177. Klipstein, K. H. (1960). Your health nd industr . New York: National Health Council. 243 Knapp, S. L. (1985). The effect of occupational stress and social support on alcohol use and abuse (Doctoral Dissertation, Southern Illinois University at Carbondale). Dissertation Abstracts International, 33(10), SECB, 3583. Korcok, M., and Seidler, G. (Eds). (197 8, February-March). Focus on alcohol and drug issues, 1(1), 4-5. Kurtz, N. R., Googins, B., & Howard, W. C. (1984). Measuring the success of occupational alcoholism programs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 4_5(1), 33-45. Kuzmits, F., & Hammonds, H. (1979, April). Rehabilitating the troubled employee. Personnel Journal, 238-250. Landy, F. J., Trumbo, D. A. (1976). Psychology of work behavior. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. Lazarus, R. S. (1974). Psychological stress and coping in adaptation to illness. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 3, 321-333. Leavitt, R. L. (1983). Employee assistance and counseling programs. New York: Community Council of Greater New York. Leaw, R. L. (1983). Social support and psychological disorder: A review. Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 3-21. LeRoux, M. (1982). Employee assistance programs: Well worth their cost, employers say. Wren—Ce, £65), 3-20. Levenson, H. (1974). Activism and powerful others: Distinctions within the concept of internal-external control. Jo m l of Person lit Assessment, 38, 377-383. Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., & Nerenz, D. (1980). The common sense representation of illness danger. In S. Rachman (Ed.), Medical Psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Pergamon. Levine, H. Z. (1983). Corporate wellness programs. P3r_s_o_p_r1el_, 30(4), 50—53. Levinson, H. (1976). The abrasive personality. Harvard Business Review, 33, 86-94. Lewis, K. A. (1981). Employee assistance programs: The current state of the art of mental health services in government and industry (Doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University). WW, 3058. (University Microfilms No. 8124944) Lin, N,, Simeone, R, S,, Ensel, W. M., & Kuo, W. (1979). Stressful life events, and illness: A model and empirical test. rn l fH lth n i IB h vi 132.912), 108-19. 244 Litwin, G. H., & Stringer, R. A., Jr. (1968). Motivation and organizational climate. Boston: Division of Research, Graduate School of Business Administration, Harvard University. Locke, E. A., Shaw, K. N., Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P. (1981). Goal setting and task performance: 1969-1980. Psychological Bulletin, %, 125-152. Lord, R. G., & Hanges, P. J. (1987). A control systems model of organizational motivation: Theoretical development and applied implications. Behavioral Scienee, 3_2, 161-178. Lovenheim, B. (1979, April 1). More care given employee’s psyches. New York Times, 1,3. Loy, J ., & Ingham, A. (1973). Play, games, and sport in the psychosocial development of children and youth. In G. Rarick (Ed.), Physical activity: Human growth and development (pp. 257—302). New York: Academic Press. Loy, J., McPherson, B., & Kenyon, G. (1978). Sport and social sgtems: A guide to the analysis, problems and literature. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Lyons, P. (1987). EAPs the only real cure. Management Review, 13(3), 38-41. Malen, A. L. (1982). Factors influencing participation in a health promotion program of exercise (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma). March, J. G., & Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. New York: Wiley. Margolis, B. L., Kroes, W., & Quinn, R. P. (1974). Job stress: An unlisted occupational hazard. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 13(10), 659-661. Marino, J. A. (1985). Identifying factors relating to employee assistance program utilization (Doctoral dissertation, George Peabody College for Teachers of Vanderbilt University). Martin, T. N ., & Schermerhorn, J. R. (1983). Work and nonwork influences on health: A research agenda using inability to leave as a critical variable. Academy of W, 8(4), 650-659. Masi, D. A. (1978). Implementation strategies in EAP’s in higher education. nf r n Pr e in 3 Au ust -9 1977 Em lo eAssistance Pro rams in Hi h r Educadm. Columbia: University of Missouri and NIAAA. Masi, D. A. (1979). Combating alcoholism in the workplace. He 1 h nd Soci I Work, 4(4), 41-59. Masi, D. A. (1982). W. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath and Company. 245 Masi, D. A. (1984). Designing employee assistance programs. New York: American Management Associations. Masi, D. & Teems, L. (1983). Employee counseling services evaluation system: Design, issues and conclusions. Evaluation and Program Planning, 3, 1-6. Matsunaga, S. (1983). The federal role in research, treatment and prevention of alcoholism. American Psychologist, 3_8, 1111-1115. Matteson, M. T., & Ivancevich, J. M. (1982). The how, what and why of stress management training. Personnel Journal, 31, 768-774. Mayer, S. J. (1988). An exploratory study of the correlates associated with a worksite health promotion intervention (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon). Dissertation Abstracts International, 33(02), SECA, 483. McClellan, K. (1982). An overview of occupational alcoholism issues for the 80’s. Jeurnal of Drug Education, 12(1), 1-27. McClellan, K. (1985). The changing nature of EAP practice. Personnel Administrator, 3_0(8), 29-37. McCroskey, J. (1982). Employers subsidize day care. Wm, 3(2). McKinlay, J. B. (1975). The help-seeking behavior of the poor. In J. Kosa and I. K. Zola (Eds), Poverty and health (pp. 224-273). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Mechanic, D. (1982). The epidemiology of illness behavior and its relationship to physical and psychological distress. In David Mechanic (Ed.), mm behavior, and help-seeking (pp. 1-24). New York, NY: Prodist. Merriman, W. J. (1984). The relationship of socialization, attitude and placement to the degree of participation in physical activity of emotionally disturbed high school students (Doctoral Dissertation, New York University). LEW W, 36(02), SECA, 374. Meyers, D. W. (1985). Employee problem preyention and counseling: A guide for We. Westport, CT: Quorum Books. Miller, E. J . (1975). Socio-technical systems in weaving, 1953-1970: A follow-up study. W, 28, 349-386. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structurle kinking. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Miller, L. (1977). A counseling program in industry: Polaroid. Social Thought, 3(1), 37— 45. 246 Minter, J. (1983). A contracted or in—house EAP? EAP Digest, 3(4), 21-22. Mitchell, R., & Hurley, D. (1981). Collaborating with natural helping networks. In B. Gottlieb (Ed.), Social networks and social support. California: Sage Publications. Molloy, D. J. (1986). Planning and implementing a worker based and participatory model for employee assistance programs (Doctoral dissertation, City University of New York). Moriyana, I., Kreuger, D., & Stamler, J. (1971). Cardiovascular Diseases in the United States. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Mullen, B., & Suls, J. (1982). "Know thyself": Stressful life changes and the ameliorative effect of private self-consciousness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 43-55. Murray, E. (1983). Human resource services needs in business and industry: Implications for the preparation of guidance personnel by universities (Doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1416. (University Microfilms No. 8318410) Myers, D. W. (1984). Establishing and building employee assistance programs. Westport, CT: Quorum Books. Myers, P. S., & Myers, D. W. (1985). Are employers conforming to standards? EAP Digest, 3, 55-63. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). 1978. Third special report to the US. Congress on alcohol and health. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). (1982). Occupational aleoholism: A review of research issues. Research Monograph No. 8. Washington, DC: NIAAA. Nelson, D., & Campbell, S. (1972, Spring). Taylorism vs. welfare work in American industry. Business History Review, 1-16. New York Executive Chamber. (I982). The 1982 report on drug abuse and alcoholism. Prepared for Governor Hugh L. Carey by Joseph A. Califano, Jr. New York: Executive Chamber. Norris, J. (1948). Cost and remedy of the alcoholic hangover in industry. Industrial Medicine and Surgery, fl, 129—132. 247 O’Bryan, G. G. (1972). The relationship between an individual’s I-E orientation and information-seeking learning and use of weight control relevant information. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Nevada). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, 33B,447B. (University Microfilms No. 72-19,541). O’Connell, J. K., & Price, J. H. (1982). Health locus of control of physical fitness in program participants. Perceptaal and Motor Skills, 5(3), 925-926. Opinion Research Corporation (1972, 1974, 1976, 1979). Executives’ knowledge, attitudes, and behavior regarding alcoholism and alcohol abuse: A report of executive caravan findings. Princeton, NJ: Opinion Research Corp. Orlandi, M. A. (1986). The diffusion and adoption of worksite health promotion innovations: An analysis of barriers. Preventive Medicine, fl, 522-536. Ozawa, M. N. (1980). Development of social services in industry: Why and how? Social Work, 23, 464-470. Palmore, E. (1971). Longevity predictors: Implications for practice. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 33, 160-164. Parcel, G. S., Nader, P. R., & Rogers, P. J. (1980). Health locus of control and health values: Implications for school health education. Health Values: Achieving High Level Wellness, 4, 32-37. Pardue, M. A. (1987). A secondary analysis of a client satisfaction survey for an internal employee assistance program (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Arlington). Pati, G. C., & Adkins, J. I. (1983). The employers role in alcoholism assistance. W, 62. 568-572. Pattison, E. M., & Kaufman, E. (Eds) (1983). Encyclopedic handbook of alcoholis_m. New York: Gardner Press. Pearlin, L. I., & Radabaugh, C. W. (1976). Economic strains and the coping functions of alcohol. American Joprnal of Sociology 82 652-663. ,—, Pelletier, K. R., & Lutz, R. (1988). Healthy people--healthy business: A critical review of stress management in the workplace. American Journal of Health Promotion, 2, 5-12. Pender, N. J. (1982). Health promotion in nursing practice. New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts. Perham,J. (1984). Companies gain in health cost fight. Dun’ Bu in M n h, 123(3), 68-74. 248 Pilisuk, M., & Minkler, M. (1985). Supportive ties: A political economy perspective. Health Education Quarterly, 12(1), 93—106. Platt, E. S. (1969). Internal-external control and changes in expected utility as predictions of the change in cigarette smoking following role-playing. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological Association, Philadelphia, 1969. Popple, P. R. (1981, June). Social work practice in business and industry, 1875-1930. Social Service Review, p. 259. Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago: Aldine. Public Health Service. (1979). Healthy people: The surgeon general’s report on health promotion and disease prevention. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Quick & Quick (1984). Organizational stress and preventive management. McGraw- Hill. Richards, C. M. B. (1988). The comparison of health motivation, health knowledge, health locus-of—control, and health protective behavior of freshmen and senior college nursing majors (Doctoral dissertation, Boston University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 3(06), SECA, 2111. Rivera, M. V.. (1984). Alcoholism and employee assistance programs in the public sector: Past, present, and future prospects (Doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Albany). Roman, P. M. (1978). Adapting employee alcoholism and assistance programs for college and university faculty. Conference Proceedin 5 Au ust7-9 1977 Em 10 ee Assistance Pregrams in Higher Education. Columbia: University of Missouri and NIAAA. Roman, P. M. (1980a, August). Employee alcoholism and assistance programs. Journal of Higher Education, 31, 135-149. Roman, P. M. (1980b). Medicalization and social control in the workplace: Prospects for the 1980s The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 13(3), 3. Roman, P. M. (1981). From employee alcoholism to employee assistance. W St di n Alcohol, 42(3), 244-272. Roman, P. M. (1983a). Employee assistance programs in Australia and the United States: Comparison of origin, structure, and the role of behavioral science research. 19mg] 3f Applied Behayioral Seienee, 19(3), 280-286. R01 R01 249 Roman, P. M. (1983b). Pitfalls of ’program’ concepts in the development and maintenance of employee assistance programs. Urban and Social Change Review, 2(1), 9-12. Roman, P. M. (1984). The environment for EAP research: Comments on structure of organizational support. In C. H. Grimes (Ed.), EAP research. An annual of research and research issues (pp. 1-10). Troy, MI: Performance Resource Press. Roman, P. M., & Trice, H. (1976). Alcohol abuse and work organizations. In B. Kissen & H. Beglieler (Eds), Biology of alcoholism (Vol. 4, pp. 445-517). New York: Plenam Press. Rosenstock, I. M. (1975). Individual responsibility for health maintenance. In S. M. Weiss (Ed.), Proceedings of the national heart and lung institute working conference on health behavior (DHEW Pub. No. NIH 76-868). Bethesda, MD: Public Health Service. Rosenstock, I. M., & Kirscht, J. P. (1979). Why people seek health care. In G. C. Stone, F. Cohen, & N. E. Adler Health psychology--a handbooszheories, applications, and challenges of a psychological approach to the health care system (pp. 161-188). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Roth, R. (1981, March/April). The EAP works: A profitable new tool for the private sector. Alc_o_holism Magazine, pp. 23-27. Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and elinical psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcements. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 83, 1-28. Rotter, J. B. (1975). Some problems and misconceptions related to the construct of internal vs. external control of reinforcement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, fl, 55-67. Rotter, J., Chance, J., & Phares, E. (1972). Applications of a Social Learning Theory of My. New York: Holt, Reinhard, and Winston. Rush, H., & Brown, J. (1971, March). The drug problem in business: A survey of business opinions and experience. The Conference Board Record, 1(3), 7-15. Sager, L. B. (1979). The corporation and the alcoholic. W3, 13(6), 79-82. Savoca, L. A. (1986). The structure and service delivery characteristics of selected employee assistance programs in the tri-state area of New York, New Jersey and Connecticut. (Doctoral dissertation, Adelphi). Scahill, J. L. (1963). Effects of secondary school health instruction on the health knowledge of college freshmen (Doctoral dissertation, State University of Iowa). 250 Scanlon, W. (1983). Trends in EAPs: Then & now. EAP Digest, 3(4), 38-41. Scanlon, W. F. (1986). Alcoholism and drug abuse in the workplace. New York: Praeger Publishers. Scheier, M. F., Fenigstein, A., & Buss, A. H. (1974). Self-awareness and physical aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, m, 264-273. Schlenger, W. E., & Hayward, B. M. (1975). Assessing the impact of occupational programs. Raleigh, NC: Human and Ecology Institute. Schmitz, H. (1982). The handbook of employee counseling programs. New York: The New York Business Group on Health. Schultz, S. K. (1981). An exploratory study of the relationship of some psychosocial factors and health beliefs to compliance with the therapeutic regimen among adolescents with cystic fibrosis. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 2867A- 2868A. Schwartz, G. E. (1978). Psychobiological foundations of psychotherapy and behavior change. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin (Eds.)., Handbook of Psychotherapy and behavior change (2nd Ed.). New York: Wiley. Schwartz, G. E. (1979). The brain as a health care system. In G. C. Stone, F. Cohen, & N. E. Adler (Eds), Health psychology--A handbook. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Seeman, M., & Evans, J. W. (1962). Alienation and learning in a hospital setting. American Sociological Review, 21(6), 772-783. Seeman, M., & Seeman, T. (1983). Health behavior and personal autonomy: A longitudinal study of the sense of control in illness. Journal of Health and Social mm. 2.4, 144-160. Seltzer, E. (1978). Locus of control and the intention to lose weight. Health Education Menographs, 3, 118-127. Serxner, S. A. (1988). Organizational environment and employee participation in worksite health promotion programs (Doctoral dissertation, University of California). D. issertatien Abstraets Internatienal, 49105), SECA, 1647. Shain, M., & Groeneveld, J. (1980). Employee assistance programs; Philosophy, theory W. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Shain, M., Suurvali, H., & Boutilier, M. (1986). H lthi r orker : Health romo ion 9W. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. Shamir, B, & Bargal, D, (1982). Occupational welfare and organizational effectiveness: Some theoretical notes. WM, 3(4), 43-52. 251 Shephard, R. J. (1985). Motivation: The key to fitness compliance. The Physician and iports Medicine, Q0), 88-101. Shetty, Y. K. (1982). Key elements of productivity improvement programs. Business Horizons, 23(2), 15-22. Shuster, P. (1978). Human resources management: Key to the future. The Personnel Administrator, _9_, 33-36. Sibley, R., & McFarland, D. (1974). A state-space approach to motivation. In D. McFarland (Ed.), Motivational control systems analysis (pp. 213-250). New York: Academic Press. Skidmore, R. A., Balsam, D., & Jones, 0. F. (1974). Social work practice in industry. Social Mrk, 12(3). Smirnow, B. (1980). Case studies in mental wellness programming. In R. Egdahl and D. Walsh (Eds), Mental wellness program for employees. New York: Springer. Smith, A. (1776). An inquiry into the nature and causes of the wealth of nations. R. H. Campbell, A. S. Skinner, & W. B. Todd (Eds). London: Oxford University Press 1976. ) Snyder, E., & Spreitzer, E. (1973). Family influence and involvement in sports. Research Quarterly, 44, 249—255. Snyder,J. J. (1989). Health psychology and behavioral medicine. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Sonstroem, R. J., & Walker, M. I. (1973). Relationship of attitudes and locus of control to exercise and physical fitness. Perceppml and Motor Skills, 33, 1031-1034. Spacapan, S. (1988). Psychosocial mediators of health status: An introduction. In S. Spacapan and S. Oskamp (Eds), The so i l s holo of health (pp. 17-29). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Sprinthall, R. C., & Sprinthall, N. A. (1981). Educational psychology; A developmental W (3rd ed.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Sta", A. & Byram, G. (1985). Cost/benefit analysis for employee assistance programs. WW, 3, 8, 55-62. Steenbarger, B. N., & Aderman, D. (1979). Objective self-awareness as a nonaversive state: Effect of anticipating discrepancy reduction. rnal of P r onalit , 41, 330- 339. Steidinger, J. (1986). Assessment of the current state of evaluation practices in employee assistance programs. Dis r i A ct Intern i n 1,46, 2637. 252 Stevens, J. R. (1970, June). It’s high time to have a drug policy. Modern Office Procedpres, 28-36. Stone, G. C. (1979). Health and the health system: A historical overview and conceptual framework. In G. C. Stone, F. Cohen, & N. E. Adler, Health psychology--a handbook: Theeries, applications, and challenges of a psychological approach to the health care system (pp. l—17). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Stone, G. C., Cohen, F., & Adler, N. E. (1979). Health psychology-~a handbook: Theories, applications, and challenges of a psychological approach to the health care system. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Storm, L. D. (1977). Solving job performance problems. Tallahassee: Human Conservation Associates. Straussner, L. A. (1985). The nature and growth of contractual EAPs. ALMACAN, 13(9), 20-23. Straussner, S. L. A. (1986). Helping troubled employees: An analysis of selected employee assistance programs under management auspices. (Doctoral Dissertation). Strickland, B. R. (1978). Internal—external expectancies and health-related behaviors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psyehology, 43, 1192-1211. Suddeth, A. B. (1984). Assessing employee use of internal and external employee assistance programs for alcohol and control group. In C. H. Grimes (Ed.), EAP researeh, Ap annual of research and researeh issues, (Vol. 1, pp. 24-36). Troy, MI: Performance Resources Press. Swanson, J. (1972). Second thoughts on knowledge and attitude effects upon behavior. ournal of School Health, 3, 363-365. Syme, S. L. (1986). Strategies for health promotion. Preventive Medicine, 13, 492-507. Taylor, F. (1911). Principles of Scientifie Management. New Norton. Taylor, M. S., Fisher, C. D., & Ilgen, D. R. (1984). Individuals’ reactions to performance feedback in organizations: A control theory perspective. In K. Rowland and J. Ferris (Eds), Research in Perseanel ana Human Resoarees Management (Vol. 2, 81-124). Greenwich, CT: J Al Press. Terborg, J. R. (1986). Health promotion at the worksite: A research challenge for personnel and human resources management. In K. M. Rowland & G. R. Ferris (Eds), Research in Persennel and Hpman Reseurces Management (Vol. 4, pp. 225- 267). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 253 Terborg, J. R. (1988). The organization as a context for health promotion. In S. Spacapan and S. Oskamp (Eds), The social psychology of health (pp. 129-174). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Tersine, R. J ., & Hazeldine, J. (1982, August). Alcoholism: A productivity hangover. Business Horizons, 68-72. Thomas, D. (1982). Who’s using the new fringes? The ALMACAN, Q00). Thomas, J. M. S. (1984). Relationship of general health motivation and dietary sodium intake (Doctoral dissertation, Texas Woman’s University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 43(12), SECB, 3775. Thoresen, R. W., Knocke, F., Roberts, L., & Pascoe, E. (1977). The University of Missouri EAP: Orientation training, referral services, evaluation. Conferenee Proceegings, August 1-3, 1976, Employee Assistance Programs in Higher Education. Columbia: University of Missouri and NIAAA. Trice, H. M. (1980, Spring). Applied research studies: Job-based alcoholism and employee assistance programs. Alcohol, Health & Research World, 4, 4-16. Trice, H. M., & Beyer, J. M. (1980). Job based alcoholism programs: Motivating problem drinkers to rehabilitation. In E. M. Pattison and E. Kaufman (Eds), _T_he American Handbook of Alcoholism. New York: Gardner Press. Trice, H. M., & Roman, P. M. (1972, 1978). Spirits and demons at work: Alcohol and ether drugs on the job. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Trice, H. M., & Schonbrunn, M. (1981). A history of job based alcoholism programs: ‘1900-1955. Journal of Drug Issues, 2, 171-198. Tyroler, H. A., Johnson, A. L., & Fulton, J. T. (1965). Patterns of preventive health behavior in populations. Journal of Health and Human Behavior, 3, 128-140. Ultsch, S. L. (1983). Motivation for voluntary regular exercise and the health benefit: An analysis of attributions and other factors (Doctoral dissertation, University of Alabama, Birmingham). United States Congress. (1978). Senate committee on human resources. Subcommittee on alcoholism and drug abuse (Aug. 17, 1978). Oceupatienal aleoholism programs under federal contracts. GPA Item No.: 1043; Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. United States Congress. (1982). Committee on Labor and Human Resources. Subcommittee on Alcoholism and Drug Abuse and Subcommittee on Employment and Productivity (July 14, 19.82) W (GPO Item No.: 1043- A, 1043- B; microfiche). Washington, D..:C US Government Printing Office. 254 U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (1919). Welfare work for employees. Urwick, L. (1956). The golden book of management. London: Newman Neame. Uyeno, C. M. (1988). Characteristics of primary care provided by employee assistance programs. (Doctoral dissertation) Vicary, J. R., & Resnik, H. (1982). Preventing drug abuse in the workplace (Drug Abuse Prevention Monograph Series). Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. Von Bertalanffy, L. (1966). General systems theory and psychiatry. In S. Arieti, (Ed.), American Handbook of Psychiatry, (Vol. 3). Basic Books. Von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. New York: Braziller. Von Wiegand, R. A. (1974, January). Advance in secondary prevention of alcoholism through the cooperative efforts of labor and management in employer organizations. Preventive Medicine, 3, 80-83. Wallston, K. A., Maides, S. A., & Wallston, B. S. (1976). Health-related information seeking as a function of health-related locus of control and health value. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 215-222. Wallston, B. S., & Wallston, K. A. (1978, Spring). Locus of control and health: A review of the literature. Health Education Monographs, 3, 107-115. Wallston, K. A., Wallston, B. S., & DeVellis, R. (1978). Development of the multidimensional health locus of control (MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs, 3, 160-170. Wallston, B. S., Wallston, K. A., Kaplan, G. D., & Maides, S. A. (1976). Development and validation of the health locus of control (HLC) scale. ournal fConsul in nd Clinieal Psychology, fl, 580-585. Warren, D. (1982). Using helping networks: A key social bond of urbanites. In D. Biegel & A. Naparstek (Eds.),Communi su orts stems and ment lh alth. New York: Springer. Washington Business Group on Health (1978). A survey of industry sponsored healt_h premetien, prevention, and edueation programs. Unpublished interim report prepared for the Department of Health, Education and Welfare, National Conference on Health Promotion in Occupational Settings. Washington, D.C.: Author. Weiner, H. J., Akabas, S. H., & Sammer, J. J. (1973). Mental health care in the worlp 91m. New York: Association Press. Weiss, R. M. (1980). D lin wi h l h li m in the work I . New York: The Conference Board. Will Wim Winl Wise] P; 255 Weissman, A. (1976). Industrial social services: Linkage technology. Social Casework, 51(1). Wells, S. E. (1988). Employee assistance programs: Profile of EAP workers at 15 major employing Orange County companies. (Master’s Thesis, California State University, Long Beach). Westrate, R. M. (1983, January). White collar Stress. Central New Jersey Monthly, pp. 66-67. White House Office of Drug Abuse Policy. (1978). Drug use patterns, eonsequences and the federal response: A policy review. Washington, D.C.: Author. Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Control and communication in the animal and the machine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Williams, A. F. (1972). Factors associated with seat belt use in families. Journal of Safety Research, 4, 133-138. Williams, T. A., Calhoun, G., & Ackoff, R. L. (1982). Stress, alcoholism, and personality. Human Relations, 33(6), 491-510. Winefield, H. R., & Peay, M. Y. (1980). Behavioural science in " ' London: George Allen & Unwin. Winkelpleck, J. M. (1984). Direction EAPs move. EAP Digest, 4(5), 18-21. Wiseman, H. (1966). Political systems: Some sociological approaches. New York: F. A. Praeger. Witte, R., & Cannon, M. (1979). Employee assistance programs: Getting top management’s support. Personnel Administrator, fl(6), 23-26;44. Woelfel, J., & Haller, A. (1971). Significant others, the self—reflexive act and the attitude formation process. American Soeiological Review, 3_6, 74—87. Wolf, K. (1982). Casefinding models to identify and refer the impaired professional. EAP Digest, 2(6), 38—39. Work. (1974, December 1). Boston Globe, pp. 1—4. WIICh, J. (1974). The employee assistance program. Center City, MN: Hazelden. W’iCh. J. (1978). Employee assistance programs: Models and alternatives. m Proeeedings, Augus; Z—9, 1272, Employee Assistance Programs in Higher Education. Columbia: University of Missouri and NIAAA. 256 Wrich, J. T. (1980a). Guidelines for developing an employee assistance pregram. Center City, MN: The Hazelden Foundation. Wrich, J. T. (1980b). The employee assistance program; Updated for the 1980’s. Minneapolis, MN: Hazelden Foundation. Wrich, J. T. (1986). The impact of substance abuse at the workplace. In H. Axel (Ed.), Corperate strategies for controlling substance abuse, (Report No. 883, pp. 11-18). New York, NY: The Conference Board, Inc. Wriston, W. B. (1982). Introduction: Worksite health promotion. Health Education Quart—$11, 9.. 7-8- Wynne, K. L. (1986). The relationship of social support contracting to worksite weight control (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina). W International, 41(11), SECA, 3991. Yankelovich, D. (1978, May). The new psychological contracts at work. Psycpplpgy M!) 46750- APPENDIX A DEFINITION OF TERMS APPENDIX A DEFINITION OF TERMS Broad-Brash EAP refers to an EAP designed to address a wide variety of employee concerns and problems, including alcoholism, drug abuse, divorce, marital discord, child rearing, stress, financial management, behavioral and psychological concerns, and legal problems. Its purpose is to reach as many workers and their dependents as possible. Comparator Mechanism is a component within the control theory model of EAP utilization which compares an individual’s goal (i.e., to remain healthy) with the individual’s current health status to determine whether a discrepancy exists. Curren; Health Status refers to the current health-related symptoms and current health condition of the employee. EAP Effeetiyepess refers to the cost-benefit evaluation used to assess the success of an EAP and help justify the disbursement of funds allocated to the implementation and administration of an EAP. Unfortunately, there is no consensus on how to measure EAP effectiveness. wage refers to an in-person visit to the EAP office or a call for an appointment, information, or a referral. (Also see Utilization/Referral Rate.) E ' n I D's r 3 refers to intrapersonal tension which may influence ineffective physical and/or emotional functioning. 257 e E G H 258 Employee Assistance Program refers to an intervention strategy designed to assist employees in resolving work and non-work related problems that impair job performance. These problems may include substance abuse, eating disorders, compulsive gambling, mental and emotional problems, financial, legal, and marital concerns. Help is provided through a variety of assessment, counseling, and referral methods—though EAP’s stress self—referrals in addition to supervisory referrals which the narrower alcohol-related programs relied on. Expectancy is a component within the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization which refers to an individual’s perception regarding his/her ability to attain his/her goal (i.e., remain healthy). External EAP refers to an intervention strategy involving counseling off the actual job site, typically provided by agencies or firms who contract with an organization. These include multi-service agencies which provide easy access to a variety of resources. mm is a component within the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization which refers to the end result an individual strives for; i.e., in the present research the goal of interest is to be healthy. This is also called the referent value. Health Locus of Control is a measure of peOple’s beliefs regarding whether their health is or is not determined by their behavior (Wallston, Wallston & DeVellis, 1978, p. 160). w refers to a company-hired counseling staff housed at the work site. MI “agement Social Support refers to "the social support provided by immediate supervisors and managers to employees who are interested in adopting and deg extl De He (Pl 259 maintaining health-related behaviors or in participating in health-related activities" (Mayer, 1988). WWW (MHLC) is defined as the degree to which individuals believe their health is determined by their own behavior; i.e., the degree to which individuals believe their health is controlled internally or externally (Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan & Maides, 1976; Wallston, Wallston & DeVellis, 1978). The MHLC expands the original HLC scale to reflect three Health Locus of Control dimensions: “internality (IHLC), powerful others (PHLC), and chance (CHLC) externality" (Wallston, Wallston & DeVilIis, 1978, p. 162). Occupational Alcoholism Programs provide services for alcoholic employees in industry, and include diagnostic, treatment, and rehabilitation programs. Problem/Troubled Employee is one whose work history is characterized by productivity problems, tardiness and absenteeism, accidents, and other on-the—job problems. Referral Seurce/Model is the strategy utilized in obtaining employee utilization of EAP services. The major models include: (I) a self-referral model where an employee voluntarily seeks assistance, and his or her participation in the EAP is held in strict confidence; (2) a supervisory-referral, confrontation model where a third party within an organization (e.g., supervisor, union, medical department) actually refers an employee whose performance is suffering to an EAP; and (3) a peer or significant others model where significant others (e.g., family, coworkers) encourage the impaired employee to seek assistance (Wolf, 1982), and where participation in the EAP is held in strict confidence (Featherston & Bednarik, 1981; Fisher, 1983). Sensor/Feed] whicl status envirc receii or ad enco: Wyni Mm ofar 10 . , (Wis learn by m. & In; W 00-11 the I‘; also I IS the 260 Sensor/Feedback is a component within the Control Theory Model of EAP Utilization which receives feedback from the environment regarding one’s present health status and symptoms, perceptions regarding past usage of an EAP, and other environmental stimuli. Social Support is "input provided by another person (or group) which moves the receiving person towards goals which the receiver desires” (Caplan et al., 1976) or activities provided by an individual’s social environment which reinforce and encourage an individual to undertake some behavior (Levy, 1980 as cited by Wynne, 1986). Input may be physical, verbal/information, or social/emotional. Social Systems are sets of "individuals who are . . . interacting with others on the basis of a minimal degree of complimentary expectations by means of, and according to . . . a shared system of beliefs, standards and means of communication” (Wiseman, 1966, p. 5). m is ”an interactional process whereby a person acquires a social identity, learns appropriate role behavior, and in general conforms to expectations held by members of the social systems to which he belongs or aspires to belong” (Loy & Ingham, 1973, p.258). Troubled/Problem Employee is one whose behavioral/medical problems adversely affect on-the-job performance or which would motivate the employee to seek help. Urilization/Referral Rates refers to the percentage of actual EAP service utilization;i.e., the ratio of EAP clients to the employee population (Myers, 1984). This rate has also been referred to as the "participation“ rate or the ”penetration“ rate, which is the rate at which employees have penetrated the EAP. 261 Wellness refers to pro-active, preventative strategies designed to prevent the development of employee problems within or outside the workplace. Examples of such programs might include seminars on nutrition, weight control, physical fitness, stress recognition and management, pre—retirement counseling, and improvement of social skills. Work-group Health Norms refers to the informal health rules and "social pressures generated by an employee’s referent work group in influencing health-related behavior" (Mayer, 1988). APPENDIX B SURVEY INSTRUMENT 262 Dear M.S.U. Employee: I am a PhD. student at M.S.U. interested in Employee Assistance Programs (EAPS). My dissertation is on factors affecting the use or non-use of one particular EAP—the one at M.S.U. To complete this research I need your participation. At Michigan State University. the Employee Assistance Program is offered as part of employees’ benefits package and is designed to deal with employees' problems and needs in both work and nonwork-related areas. You. as an employee. are eligible to use its services if the need arises. My research is being conducted with the approval of M.S.U.’s Employee Assrstance Program. The purpose of this letter is to ask you (0 complete the attached survey. I really need your assistance in completing this survey because I have only sent this to a randomly selected sample of M.SLU. employees. To encourage you to volunteer to participate in the study. I will pay you $5.00 upon your completion and return of the survey in the enclosed envelope wuhin twp weeks. It should only take you about a half hour to complete. It is critical that I receive responses from two groups of individuals-those who have previously used the EAP and those who have not. Therefore, it is very important to me that you respond. Please be assured your responses will be kept completely confidential. I will need your name and CAMPUS address on the next page so I know where to send your payment. However, as soon as I receive your survey. this page will be separated from your survey and you will be paid. I will have no way of matching survey responses to specific individuals. (If you do not wrsh to provide your name and do not wish (0 be paid. then you can return your survey Without this data.) The only other identification on the survey IS the letter "A” or ”B" which identifies those who have or have not used the EAP services at the university. I thank you in advance for your time in completing the survey.‘ If I don‘t receive a response from you in [W0 weeks. I will send a follow-up letter. Obvrously, 1f the costs of the follow-up can be avoided. I would appreciate II. Please return the survey to me in the enclosed envelope via U.S. MAIL as soon as possible. I feel the research subject is an important one and I need your responses to continue my work on this topic. Sincerely, Thomas Helma 3:21;“? Stampmn M.S.U. EAP Coordinator . . In em ' Iunde behaviors. I only to be us Assistance P‘ In ext enclosed en! confidential. survey, I out With specific NAME (Ple CAMPUS ; to send pay SOCIAL s 263 INFORMED CONSENT FORM " I understand the purpose of this research is to study faCtors influencing health attitudes and behaviors. I am participating voluntarily. I understand that the Group Identification (A or B) is only to be used to match my responses to either the group that has used or not used an Employee Assistance Program in the past. In exchange for my participation. I will be paid $5.00 when I return the survey in the enclosed envelope within two weeks. I understand that my responses will be kept completely confidential. My name must be provided only so payment can be sent to me. Once I return the survey, I understand my name will be detached so there will be no way to match survey responses with specific individuals. NAME (Please Print): CAMPUS ADDRESS .______——————-—-—-—— to send payment to: SOCIAL SECURITY NUMBER: (Required in order to pay you from a research fund) INSTRU 264 SURVEY GROUP SURVEY ON HEALTH BELIEFS & BEHAVIORS INFLUENCING USAGE OF EMPLOYEE ASSISTANCE PROGRAMS INSTRUCTIONS: This survey asks about your beliefs regarding your health and your experiences pertaining to specific health behaviors. It is important that you think about each statement and answer it honestly. There are no right or wrong answers. Please try to answer each item independently when you are making your choice—do nor be influenced by your previous choices. Answer all queStions using only the response choices given. Your answers will be kept completely confidential. Once each survey is completed and returned, your name will be removed from the survey so it will not be possible to match responses to any specific individual. PLEASE TURN THE PAGE TO BEGIN THE SURVEY! PART 265 PART I This section is designed to determine the way in which different people view certain important health-related issues. Each item is a belief Statement with which you may agree or disagree. Refer to the scale below and for each item circle the number that represents the uncut to which you disagree or agree with the statement. (For example. if you ”slightly disagree" with a statement. circle the number 3 on your answer sheet.) Please circle only one number per item. Remember that this is a measure of your personal beliefs—obviously there are no right or wrong answers. Try to respond to each item independently—do not be influenced by your previous choices. It is important that you respond according to your actual beliefs and mm according to how you feel you should believe. or how you think someone wants you to believe. (SD) (MD) . (SD) (SL) (MA) (SA) Strongly Moderately Slightly Slightly Moderately Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Agree Agree Agree 1 3 3 4 5 6 . so MD so SLMASA 1. If I get sick, it is my own behavior which determines how soon I will get well again ...................................... I 2 3 4 S 6 2. No matter what I do. if I am going to get sick, I will get sick ............ I 2 3 4 5 6 3. Having regular contact with my physician is the best way for me to avoid illness ........................................ I 2 3 4 5 6 4. Most things that affect my health happen to me by accident ............ 1 2 3 4 S 6 5. Whenever I don’t feel well. I should consult a medically trained professional .......................................... I 2 3 4 5 6 6. [am in control of my health .................................... I 2 3 4 5 6 7. My family has a lot to do with my becoming sick or staying healthy ...... I 2 3 4 S 6 8 When I get sick, I am to blame .................................. I 2 3 4 5 6 9. Luck plays a big pan in determining how soon I will recover from an illness .............................................. l 2 3 4 5 6 10. Health professionals control my health ............................ 1 2 3 4 5 6 II. My good health is largely a matter of good fortune ................... l 2 3 4 5 6 12. The main thing which affects my health is what I myself do ............ I 2 3 4 5 6 13. If I take care of myself. I can avoid illness ......................... 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. When I recover from an illness. it’s usually because other people (e.g.. doctors, nurses. family, friends) have been taking good care of me ....... I 2 3 4 5 6 15. No matter what I do. I'm likely to get sick ......................... I 2 3 4 5 6 16. If it’s meant to be. I will Stay healthy ............................. l 2 3 4 5 6 17. [fl take the right actions. I can stay healthy ........................ I 2 3 4 S 6 18. Regarding my health. I can only do what my doctor 1 2 3 4 - 6 tells me to do ..... ' .......................................... PART Plo you 949*:— :1: a: < 266 PART II Please rate how strongly you agree or disagree with the following statements with respect to your life in general. Circle the appropriate number usmg the scale below. (SD) (D) (U) (A) (SA) Strongly Strongly Disagree Disagree Uncertain Agree Agree 1 2 4 ' :—T so 0 SA I. I’m always trying to figure myself out ............................. 1 2 3 4 3 2. I’m concerned about my style of domg things ....................... I 2 3 4 5 3. Generally. I’m n0t very aware of myself ........................... 1 2 3 4 5 4. I reflect about myself a lot ..................................... I 2 3 4 5 5. I’m concerned about the way I present myself ...................... I 2 3 4 5 6. I’m often the subject of my own fantasies .......................... I 2 3 4 5 7. I never scrutinize myself ....................................... I 2 3 4 5 8. I’m self-conscious about the way I look ........................... I 2 3 4 5 9. I’m generally attentive to my inner feelings ........................ 1 2 3 4 S 10. I usually worry about making a good impression ..................... I 2 3 4 5 11. I’m constantly examining my own motives ......................... I 2 3 4 5 12. One of the last things that I do before I leave the house is look in the mirror I 2 3 4 5 13. I sometimes have the feeling that I’m off somewhere watching myself ..... I 2 3 4 5 14. I'm concerned about what other people think of me .................. I 2 3 4 5 IS. I’m alert to changes in my mood ................................ I 2 3 4 5 16. I’m usually aware of my appearance .............................. I 2 3 4 5 17. I’m aware of the way my mind works when I work through a problem . . . . I 2 3 4 5 PART III This section examines how healthy you have been throughout your life. These questions cover your health while growing up and as an adult. For each statement below. please respond by circling either 0 for NO or I for YES. & fl 1. Have you ever been considered a sickly person? .......................... O I 2. Have you ever had a weight problem (overweight or underweight)? .......... O I 3. Have you frequently experienced injuries (sprains. burns. poisoning. dislocations. serious back problems. etc.) ............................... O I 4. Have you ever had a tumor. cancer. cyst. or growth? ...................... 0 I 5. Have you ever had major surgery? .................................... O 1 6. Have you ever had repeated painful headaches (e.g., migraine) or any types of fits (seizures. epilepsy. convulsions)? .................................. 0 I 7. Have you ever had any serious trouble with your hearing or vision? ........... 0 I 8. Have you ever suffered from high blood pressure or hypertension? ............ 0 I 9. Have you ever had diabetes or a gallbladder or liver disease? ................ 0 I PART II. IO. Have ll. Have longt I2. Have pneu 13. Have I4. Have IS. M PART IV The n an El exper marit: variet servic Please ._. - My lri Pressu "ll go 2' Ithinl 3° lwoul embar 4- My fri befOre 3' MY fat [Othe 6- Going take [c 7' [think SCCkinl ' [WOUII SeWitte ‘ Membi Would ‘ l“’Oult the EA 267 PART III (Continued) 929. YES 10. Have you ever been diagnosed as having high cholesterol levels? ............. 0 I I1. Have you ever had any kind of trouble with your blood (blood dysfunction. bleed longer from cuts than most peOple. anemia. excessive bleeding with periods. etc.)? 0 I l2. Have you ever had any serious respiratory problems (lungs. bronchitis. pneumonia. frequent heavy chest colds. etc)? ............................ 0 I 13. Have you had frequent infecrions (kidney, urinary, throat. skin. etc)? .......... 0 I 0 I 14. Have you had problems with allergies? ................................. 15. @W MANY serious health problems have you had not listed above? Write number: PART IV The questions in this section refer to an Employee Assistange Prgggam (EAP). M.S.U. provides an EAP as a benefit to all its employees to help them resolve any type of problem they may be experiencing (e.g., personal. emotional. physical. nutritional. legal. subStance abuse. financial. marital, child-care. etc.). M.S.U.’s EAP is located in a building on campus and provides a variety of services which include diagnosis of the problem. short-term and long-term counseling services. seminars on a variety of topics. and referral to community resources if needed. Please circle the one best response for each item using the following scale: (SD) (MD) (SD) (SL) (MA) (SA) Strongly Moderately Slightly Slightly Moderately Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 3 6 I. My friends outside the workplace have supported. encouraged. or so MDSDSLMA SA pressured me to use the EAP services OR they would need to support my going to the EAP for me to seek assistance at the EAP ............. l 2 3 4 5 6 2. I think the EAP is too inaccessible or inconvenient for me to use ........ I 2 3 4 S 6 3. I would never use the EAP because I’m too shy or would be embarrassed to discuss any problem I might have with a counselor ....... 1 2 3 4 J 6 4- My friends outside the workplace would need to have utilized an EAP before I would go to an EAP to help resolve any problem I may have I 2 3 4 5 6 5- My family would need to support my using the EAP before I would go to the EAP to help resolve any problem I may have .................. I 2 3 4 5 6 6. Going to the EAP would interfere with my Other activities/ take too much time .......................................... I 2 3 4 5 6 7. I think pe0ple should handle their problems on their own without seeking any type of counseling .................................. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. I would never use the EAP because it doesn’t provide the types of - services or staff I need to help me with any problems I might have ....... 1 2 3 4 a 6 9- Members of my family would need to have used an EAP before I would use any of the EAP services available ........................ I 2 3 4 5 6 10. I would nor use the EAP because either I don‘t know how to contact 7 3 4 5 6 the EAP or I just don’t know anything about the EAP ................ I PART IV ( (S Strt Dis ~ II. My spo assistan 12. My clo: why I f utilize : I3. lwoult NOT k I4. My sup me to t 15. The un to use I 16. The on my con if My son resolvii 13. It is in would use any '9- My sup Wouldn 3‘1 I woulc lo the | RI. M.S.U. EAP bt 22' i Woulc your [c ‘ I WOUIt find Ou to MS. 25' My Coy “OI list: 26' MY cov would - 27' i Woult infOnm ) ‘ i Woult is {1 gm ‘ [ wOult ever 1 29 268 PART IV (Continued) (SD) (MD) (SD) (SL) (MA) (SA) Strongly Moderately Slightly Slightly Moderately Strongly Disagree Disagree Disagree Agree Agree Agree 1 2 3 4 5 6 I 11. My spouse/significant other would have to pressure me into seeking souo so SLMASA assistance at the EAP before I would use any of the EAP’s services ...... I 2 3 4 5 6 12. My close acquaintances would need to encourage me and undersrand why I felt I needed to seek assistance at the EAP before I would utilize any of‘the EAP’s services ................................. I Z 3 4 5 6 13. I would not use M.S.U.’s EAP because I am afraid the EAP Staff would NOT keep my problem confidential .............................. I 2 3 4 5 6 I4. My supervisor at work would' need to encourage. pressure. or require me to use M.S.U.’s EAP before I would seek assistance at the EAP office . . I 2 3 4 5 6 15. The union I belong to at work would have to encourage employees to use the EAP before I would ever use any of its services ............. I 2 3 4 S 6 16. The only way I would use M.S.U.’s EAP is if I knew one or more of ‘ my coworkers had already used the EAP services at some time .......... l 2 3 4 5 6 17. My supervisor would need to have used the EAP for me to use it for resolving any problem I may have ............................... I 2 3 4 5 6 18. It is important to me to do what my coworkers think is best so they would need to encourage/support my using the EAP before I would use any of its services ......................................... 1 2 3 4 5 6 19. My supervisor doesn't care about my health or my problems so I wouldn’t go to M.S.U.’s EAP even ifl had a problem ................ I 2 3 4 5 6 20. I would be too embarrassed to have anyone at work find out I went to the EAP ................................................ I 2 3 4 5 6 21. M.S.U. would have to promote and encourage employees to go to the EAP before I would use any of its services ......................... 1 2 3 4 5 6 22. I would be afraid my coworkers at work would think I was weak if I wenttOtheEAP ........................................... 12345 6 23. I would not use the EAP because I am afraid someone at work would find out and it would hurt my career/job at M.S.U. ................. I 2 3 4 5 6 24. M.S.U. doesn’t care about my health or my problems so I wouldn‘t go to M.S.U.’s EAP even ifI had a problem .......................... I 2 3 4 5 6 25- My coworkers don’t care about my health or my problems so I would not listen to their advice about going to the EAP ................... I 2 3 4 5 6 26. My coworkers think peOpIe should handle their own problems so I . would never go to the EAP ................................... I 2 3 4 3 6 27. I would not go to the EAP because M.S.U. doesn’t keep employees - informed about the services available ............................ I 2 3 4 a 6 28. I would contact M.S.U.’s EAP if I had a problem because I feel the EAP _ is a good benefit provided to employees .................... . ...... 1 2 3 4 3 6 29. I would prefer using counselors/resources outside of the workplace if I 7 3 4 5 6 I ever had a problem which required counseling ................... ._. Ix) o 269 PART V .... [Q .L» w. U: 0* an Please circle the one best response (letter) to each question. . Looking back to when vou were growing up, how would you describe your family‘s practice of healthy behaviors (exercismg, sleeping, eating, seeking medical aid when necessary, etc.)? a. It was a very important part of our daily lives b. It was moderately important to us c It was somewhat important. but only carried out haphazardly d: It was not seen as having priority Looking back to W, how w0uld you describe your mew; practice of healthy behaviors (exercismg, healthy diet and sleeping habits)? . It was a very important part of their lives It was moderately important It was somewhat important . It was not at all important . ape—re How w0uld you currently describe your family‘s practice of healthy behaviors (exercising, sleeping, eating, seeking medical aid when necessary. etc.)? a. It is a very important part of their lives b. It is moderately important It is somewhat important . It does not seem to be important . How would you cgagntly describe your friends’ practice of healthy behaviors (exercising, healthy diet and sleeping habits)? a. It is a very important part of their lives b. It is moderately important c. It is somewhat important d. It does not seem to be important . How frequently would you say you visited a health professional (medical doctor. dentist. health clinic. hospital, etc.) while you were growing up? Never . Seldom . Often . Very often CCU? . Which of the following would best describe the seeking of health assistance by members of your family was (Parents, spouse. etc.)? a. They go to a doctor for regular check-ups as well as any time they feel ill b- They only go to the docror when they feel they have something wrong c. They seldom go to the doctor even when they are in pain d. They never go to the doctor or seek any medical assistance e. I don‘t know 270 PART V (Continued) 7. Do you feel your parents/family were good at handling stress and emotional problems in their lives when vou were growing LL12? a. Most of the time b. Some of the time c. Rarely d. Never e. Not sure . Which of the following would best describe the seeking of health assistance by your friends today? a. They go to a doctor for regular check-ups as well as whenever they feel ill b. They only go to the doctor when they feel they have something wrong c. They seldom go to the doctor even when they are in pain d. They never go to the doctor or seek any medical assistance e. I don’t know Did any of your family members or friends ever seek professional assistance/counseling. to help cope with their emotional problems (such as marital or child-rearing problems. depressmn. jOb stress. handling grief, etc.) while vou were growing ug? a. Yes "b. No c. Not sure . How often did members of your family take part in regular exercise/sports while ygu wgrg gzgwing up? :1. Never b. Not often c. Often d. Very often . How often do members of your current family (spouse. children, parents. brOthers, Sisters. etc.) mm take part in regular exercise/sports? a. Never b. N0t often c. Often d. Very often . . . . 7 . How often do your current friends take part in gguju; exerCise/sports. 21. Never b. Not often c. Often d. Very often ' ' ' ll fit? - DOCS your immediate supervisor exerCise or appear to value being physxca y a. Yes b. No c. Don’t know PAI Oa‘plc H- 16. Ii 271 PART V (Continued) 14. U. How often did your friends encourage you I0 maintain healthy behaviors (proper sleep patterns, no smoking, limited alcohol. no non-prescription drugs, etc.) while you were growing uu? 21. Never b. Not often c. Often (1. Very often . Do you feel your family members or friends ever abused prescription or non-prescription drugs whil ou were owin u ".7 a. Yes b. No c. Not sure . In general. how strong are your current social ties with your family and friends? a. Very strong b. Mostly strong c. Somewhat strong d. Not very strong . How often do your current family members and/or friends encourage you to participate in exercise/sports? a. Never b. Not often c. Often d. Very often PART VI ,_4 .N . Compared to other persons your age. what do you DESIRE . . . . . .. . .7 . . k ou This section examines different health deSires of indiwduals. The followmg questions as y about your DESIRED health goals and behaviors IF YOU COULD CHOOSE THESE TODAY. There are no right or wrong answers. Please respond honestly to what you really DESIRE regarding your health, not what you think might be a correct response. by Circling the one appropriate response. your general health to be? :1. Far ahead of the average b. Somewhat above average c. Average or same as others d. Somewhat below average e. Way below average If you could select your level of exercise. what would you DESIRE yolur‘cucrtiéeg level of aerobic exercise to be per week (e.g., biking. jogging, SWImmmgv aerobics ‘- 35“ ' ' Less than once Once . Twice . Three or four times . Five or more times 0’!” 00.0 10 PAR IlficftndLLCd at)“ (Lambnmiflmnmtiu 1%. 272 PART VI (Continued) 3. a? 999? How many times per week would you LIKE to make a conscientious effort to manage your stress by utilizing progressive relaxation. exercise. religion. music, or Other stress reducrion techniques? ' 0-1 time per week 2-3 times per week 4-5 times per week 6-7 times per-week 9‘?“ Considering your height and body build, how many pounds within your ideal weight would you LIKE to fall? . Within 2-5‘ pounds (either over or under) Within 10 pounds (either over or under) Within 15 pounds (either over or under) Within 20 pounds (either over or under) Within 25 pounds (either over or under) It’s OK to be more than 25 pounds from my ideal weight (either over or under) 0.9g» e9 Which statement most closely describes your DESIRED daily eating pattem? a. Eating snack foods whenever I feel hungry (potato chips. soda pop. cookies. candy, pastry, etc.) Eating one balanced meal per day and eating snack foods at other times during the day Eating two balanced meals per day and eating snack foods at Other times during the day Eating three balanced meals per day and eating snack foods at other times during the day Eating three balanced meals per day and not snacking What stress level do you DESIRE to achieve in your personal/home life? a. I would like to be completely free of stress b. A minimal amount of stress is OK c. A moderate amount of stress is OK d. A high level of stress is OK Which statement best describes how you would LIKE to feel about anxiety? :1. It never bothers me to frequently feel anxious/uptight b. Feeling anxious/uptight occasionally does not bother me c. I dislike feeling anxious/uptight even occasionally (1. I hate ever feeling anxious/uptight and wish I would never have these feelings Which statement best describes your DESIRED level of cigarette smoking behavior? I would like to have no desire to smoke at all I would like to smoke I/2 pack or less per day I would like to smoke between 1/2 and 1 pack per day I would like to smoke from I to 1/2 packs per day I would like to smoke over l-I/Z packs but less than 2 packs per day I would like to smoke 2 packs or more per day change 11 PART VI 9. Which 273 PART VI (Continued) 0 "Gs-noun 0-D . Which choice best describes the consumption of alcoholic beverages which you DESIRE? . 0 drinks per day (I would like to never drink) . I drink or less per day . 2 drinks per day . 3 drinks per day . 4 drinks per day 2 drinks or less per weekday. and more than I drink per day on weekends . None of the above 10. How many cups of caffeinated beverages (coffee. tea, cola, etc.) do you DESIRE to drink per day? - F”? .... ..: P a. b c. d Less than 1 cup 1-3 cups c. 4—6 cups d. 7 or more cups . Which best describes your DESIRED drug use pattern (over the counter. prescription. and non- rescription drugs)? Use drugs I want whenever I want . Use drugs I feel I need while following common sense Use only medically required drugs exactly as directed . Rarely use drugs of any kind 12. How often do you DESIRE to have a medical checkup? 00.00”” . Never or only when something is wrong . Only for Pap tests or other regular checks . Every 3-5 years . At least every 2 years . At least every year 12 '0‘) 274 PART VII [0 This section presents a liSt of problems or concerns which you may currently be experiencing or may have experienced during the past year. There are three types of responses you should make regarding each problem or area of concern. Some of the quesuons refer to an Employee Assistance Program (EAP), which was defined in the previous section. The three scales to be used in this section of the survey are: CURRENT PROBLEMS SCALE In the FIRST column following each statement. please rate your current status regarding each of the health concerns or problem areas listed. Use the following scale and circle your best response under the ”CURRENT PROBLEMS" column. The items refer to ygu_r health unless otherwise n0ted. EXAMPLE I: If you feel your GENERAL PHYSICAL HEALTH is "Poor“ then you should circle the number "2" for question Ia. EXAMPLE 2: If you feel you have no BLOOD OR ANEMIA PROBLEMS then you should circle the number "5" for "Excellent/No Problem" for question 13a. (V?) (P) ~ (A) (G) (15) Very Poor/ Poor Average Good Excellent/ Have a Problem No Problem I 2 3 4 S DESIRED CHANGE SCALE In the 5% column following each item, please indicate the extent you would LIKE to change your health or improve your health in each of the problem areas by using the following scale and marking your responses under the "DESIRED CHANGE“ column. EXAMPLE: If you desire a "Very Large Change" in your current "GENERAL PHYSICAL HEALTH" then you should circle the number "5" for question lb. (N0) (SL) (M0) (L0) (VL) No Change Slight Moderate Large Very Large Desired Change Desired Change Desired Change Desired Change Desired I 2 3 4 5 EXPECTANCY SCALE In the m column following each item, please respond whether you believe going to an Employee Assistance Program (EAP) would help you either eliminate or decrease the problem liSted. regardless of whether you currently are experiencing that problem or not. Use the scale below. EXAMPLE: If you believe the EAP would be "Excellent/Help a great deal" in handling your "GENERAL PHYSICAL HEALTH“ problems. then circle "5" for question 1c. (V?) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very Poor Poor Average Good Excellent 1 2 3 4 5 (EAP would be no help) (EAP would help a l0t) 13 PART V CURREh (VP) Very P001 Have Prol .W >1 9‘ .w 5— 5.... .~.—- 0-D 5" (D "I 275 PART VII (Continued) EXPECTANCY THAT EAP WOULD HELP SCALE: (VP) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very Poor Poor Average Good Excellent 1 2 3 4 5 (EAP would be no help) (EAP would help a lOt) DESIRED CHANGE SCALE: (NO) (SL) (MD) (LG) (VL) No Slight Moderate Large Very Large Change Change Change Change Change DeSired Desired Desired Desired Desired l 2 3 4 5 CURRENT PROBLEMS SCALE: (VP) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very Poor/ Poor Average Good Excellentj Have Problem No Problem I 2 3 4 5 CURRENT DESIRED EXPECT ANCY THAT PROBLEMS CHANGE AN EAP WOUID HELP W W W I. General physical health (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 2. Cancer/cysts/growths (a) 1 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 3. Serious backaches (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 5 4. Other serious aches (a) 1 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 5. Weight problem (a) I 2 3 4 S (b) I 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 5 6. Blood pressure/hypertension (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 7. Cholesterol level (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 3 (c) l 2 3 4 5 8.Diabetes (a)12345 (b)l2345 (c)12345 9. Cardiovascular/heart problem (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 S (c) l 2 3 4 5 10. Respiratory problem(s) (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 3 (c) I 2 3 4 5 11. Vision or hearing (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 3 12.Ulcer(s) (a) 12345 (b)12345 (c)12345 l3. InfeCtion(s) (a) l 2 3 4 S (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 3 14. Blood problems/anemia (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) I 2 3 4 S (c) l 2 3 4 5 15. Handling stress (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 3 16. Legal problems (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 l7. Marital/significant (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 3 (c) I 2 3 4 5 Other problems (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) I 2 3 4 3 (c) l 2 3 4 5 18. Financial problem(s) (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 3 3 4 5 (C) 1 3 3 4 5 l4 PART VII RRENT CU (VP) Very ‘Poor/ Hill/C Pl’ObIl I \ 19 Caring ; 20- Child c; 31- Family/l 22- DIVOrc¢ 1 ' Dealing 2' Bumoui 1' Depress 6» Suicidal - Anxietyi 28' TrOuble 29' Feel alit - Feel insi 3]. Proper; 32' Eating d (aflOrexi 33‘ Regular 34' AlCOhol 35» Use of d 276 PART VII (Continued) EXPECTANCY THAT EAP WOULD HELP SCALE: (V?) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very Poor Poor Average Good Excellent I 2 3 4 5 (EAP would be no help) (EAP would help a lot) DESIRED CHANGE SCALE: (N0) (SL) l MD) (LG) (VL) No Slight Moderate Large Very Large Change Change Change Change Change Desired Desired Desired Desired Desired I 2 3 4 5 CURRENT PROBLEMS SCALE: (VP) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very'Poor/ Poor Average Good Excellent/ Have Problem No Problem 1 2 3 4 5 CURRENT DESIRED EXPECTANCY THAT PROBLEMS CHANGE AN EAP WOULD HELP . w W ELLA—(LE I9. Caring for aged (parents. etc.) (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 20. Child care problems (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 5 21. Family/parenting problems (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) I 2 3 4 5 (c) 1 2 3 4 5 22. Divorce/separation (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) I 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 5 23. Dealing with death/loss (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) 1 2 3 4 S 24. Burnout/mental fatigue (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) I 2 3 4 5 (C) l 2 3 4 5 25. Depressed/unhappy feeling (a) l 2 3 4 5 '(b) l 3 3 4 5 (C) 1 2 3 4 5 26. Suicidal feelings (a) 1 2 3 4 S (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) 1 2 3 4 5 27. Anxiety/phobias (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) 1 2 3 4 5 28. Trouble with relatives (a) 1 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 2 3 4 5 (C) 1 3 3 4 5 29. Feel alienated/withdrawn (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) l 2 3 4 5 30. Feel insecure (a) I 2 3 4 S (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 S 3]. Proper nutrition (3) l 2 3 4 S (b) I 2 3 4 5 (c) I 2 3 4 5 32. Eating disorders " (anorexia. bulimia) (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 2 3 4 5 (c) 1 2 3 4 5 33. Regular exercise (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (C) I 2 3 4 5 34. Alcohol useJabuse (a) 1 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (c) 1 2 3 4 S 35. Use of drugs/medications (a) I 2 3 4 5 (b) l 2 3 4 5 (C) I 2 3 4 5 15 PART CURR (Vi Very}! Havel 36~ Sit 37. At Please 1 [hm YOI List: I‘ll PLEAs 277 PART VII (Continued) EXPECTANCY THAT EAP WOULD HELP SCALE: (VP) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very Poor Poor Average Good Excellent I 2 3 4 S (EAP would be no help) (EAP would help a lot") DESIRED CHANGE SCALE: (NO) (SL) (MD) (LG) (VL) No Slight Moderate Large Very Large Change Change Change Change Change Desired ' Desired Desired Desired Desired l 2 3 4 3 CURRENT PROBLEMS SCALE: (VP) (P) (A) (G) (E) Very'Poor/ Poor Average Good Excellent/ I-Iave Problem No Problem I 2 3 4 5 CURRENT DESIRED EXPECT ANCY THAT PROBLEMS CHANGE AN EAP WOULD HELP we W W 36. Smoking habits (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) I 2 3 4 3 (c) I 2 3 4 3 37. Absences from work (a) l 2 3 4 5 (b) 1 3 3 4 3 (C) 1 - 3 4 3 utritional. emotional. etc.) nOt mentioned above Please 1' lth roblems ( h sical. n m below any other hea p p y blems. please continue with PART VIII on the that you may be experiencing. If you have no Other pro following page. List: ‘ ' 2345 38. a12343 (b)l234a (c)I.. _ 39. (312345 (6)1234) ((312343 40. (a)12345 (b)12345 (@1234: PLEASE CONTINUE WITH PART VIII 16 PAR' [Epab I l Elbnmde. 3m HCnminm‘Mithsiu gin I,“ I I .l .J 278 PART VIII !~) This section focuses on any previous experience you have had with an EAP and your attitude toward utilizing an EAP. Please circle the appropriate letter for each queStion. . Do you have an opportunity to utilize other assistance services Other than those at M.S.U.’S EAP that are either free or affordable (e.g., spouse's EAP, church counselors. psychiatrist. psychologist. etc.)? a. Yes b. No How willing would you be to use M.S.U.‘s EAP if you needed the services? Very willing Willing Neither willing nor unwilling Unwilling Very unwilling 9999'? Have you used any of the following resources for help with a problem that has bOthered you? Circle ALL THAT APPLY. EAP counselor-either at M.S.U.'S EAP or an EAP where you may previously have worked Mental health center (other than EAP counselor) Private counselor/psychologist Psychiatrist Clergy . Family member (circle which: spouse. child. parent, brOther, Sister. Other ____) Fdend . Self-help group (Alcoholics Anonymous. Parents Anonymous. weight-control group, etc.) Work supervisor Other. specify: . 9' f" *r‘r'sqs ran 0.9 If you HAVE used ANY EAP in the past. please respond to the following questions in this seCtion (continue with quesfion 4 below). . . If you HAVE NOT ever used an EAP. please continue Wlth PART IX on page 19. Circle the one response which best describes the improvement you experienced in your problem situation as a result of contacung the EAP: a. My problem was resolved b. I can see great improvement c. I can see some improvement d. I cannot see any improvement e. My situation has become worse f. I didn’t contact the EAP referral Was the problem you sought assistance on affecting your )Ob performance? a. Yes b. No c. Don’t know 17 279 PART VIII (Continued) 6. 10. To what extent do you think your job performance improved as a result of contacting the EAP? a. To a great extent b. To some extent c. No change d. Deteriorated somewhat e. Deteriorated a IOt How satisfied were you with the EAP? Very satisfied Satisfied Neither satisfied nor dissatisfied Dissatisfied Very dissatisfied 0’? {no.0 Would you recommend the EAP to others? I. Yes 2. No - If no, why not? If there had been no EAP where you work. would you have sought some other assistance on , your own at that time? a. Yes, I would have b. I probably would have c. Uncertain (I. Probably not e. No How do you feel about using the EAP’S services again? . Very willing Willing Neither willing nor unwilling Unwilling Very unwilling 9999's . Who initially referred you to the EAP? Self~referral Supervisory-referral . . ‘ Significant Other in personal life referral (family member. close friend. spouse) Coworker referral ‘ Union (official union representative) f. Other. please specify: L SPF-PS" . How long would you estimate the problem(s) which brought you to the EAP had been affecting you or your job before you contacted the EAP? a. Less than 6 months b. 6 months to I year c. Over 1 but less than 3 years d. 3 years or more 18 .).. THAI PLEA PAYh 280 PART [X Please answer the following questions about yourself for background and analyses purposes. Remember. your responses will be confidential and anonymous once you return the survey and your name is detached. 1. What is your gender? a. Male b. Female 2. What is your race or ethnic background? White Black Hispanic - American Indian/Alaskan Asiaanacific Islander Other ”can?!” 3. What is your educational level? a. Some or no high school b. High school graduate c. Tradelvocational school . (1. Some college e. Undergraduate college degree f. Graduate college degree 4. What type of job position do you hold at M.S.U.? a. Service or maintenance worker b. Skilled labor/trade c. Clerical d. Technical e. Professional t. Administrator/manager g. Other What is your current marital Status? a. Single (or divorced. widowed) with no dependents/children b. Single (or divorced. widowed) with dependents/children c. Married with no dependents/children (I. Married with dependents/children .Ui THANK YOU FOR Y PLEASE RETURN IT VIA W IN THE ENCLOSE PAYMENT! 6. What is your family income? Less than 39.999 510.000 to $19,999 320.000 to 829.999 330.000 to 339.999 340.000 to $49,999 $50,000 to $59,999 $60,000 or more 9‘? eraser» 7. In what age group are you? Under 25 26-30 31-35 36—40 41-45 46-50 51-55 56—60 61 or over cassette??? 8. How long have you been employed at M.S.U.? a. Less than 1 year b. At leaSt l but less than 3 years c. At least 3 but less than 5 years d. At least 5 but less than 8 years c. 8 years or more OUR PARTICIPATION! YOU HAVE COMPLETED THIS SURVEY! D ENVELOPE TO RECEIVE YOUR 19 281 If there are any comments you wish to make about the questionnaire or any aspect of the questionnaire administration process. please feel free to do so below. Thank you. 20 f MICHIGAN STIQTE UNIV. LIBRARIES . _ ,1. (I!“(HIHHIIHIIIHIIIIl"IWIHIIIWIHIIHIHHII ’7 31293009103650 ‘