- '\ val)! ‘J "I: x. ., “an :- «in f E {1,424 ' 1 V ’7 LA“ can,» 1 .m i 4. 31.1”". “.1 a “(a M «1:.I .4 . mu: 1-11.: n ”w- 41-7122“ ,. ‘J— " “£3. -\.;.x=.: I f Imnnaief‘ a —. 21. y: a}: "3:3“?5'5 9Q q; x *‘2‘ fi“’§‘§‘ “’3 11%;- gm; “’1“: 1:” Max-1 .1" ' ‘ M .33.“.441‘J - u" 223‘s x 1 u “fizz? , £111: 3' x ' an: M, ..4 h \. 11.3473“ «3.0: -‘ ‘ 1 .- u. Ar m.» r . wag: 1.. -‘:;'M‘v'~’fi *u 7 14. . at. £125 ‘ . .isis‘xtz‘ima‘s 13:. {f a. “9.? §- {31:17. ‘r m 13‘ .v 0 .13.]. .37: hr a. _ A , , "ZHRT ; 1'2"" .9 1-3.;9 LY. :- :.,‘::.r: u. v ‘r_ ~ " v5.3: 'Zn ‘3': 1.:- 1 ”.34.; 14w:- '71:; :::~ ji’LLrfl» “MEL: :1. ii;., 4 2:: 2.. qua- 'novflwq . .:1.--ummu—-c “ 414*? «$1., -Ao..'. N -K .11.-..7. , I. .- :v-t- #Qrwo'r v“ ” Woo—w «.- ' W"? i‘...g/L .3911 :3: ’13:}: - :5- .gmm ~,,.. f-‘m «n: ”H!” ”"35. ‘ 'wvaw...?;....m . 1... :wmvu A'tcfi-m: ’I‘bonnu:tm m...” we“ -——'*1IO~O on... on. ...~~rm.« fins-2.1:...“ cw-.- a W» M32...” ' :v—n. ‘ Hl_\1lk ‘ llllllllllllllllllllllllllllllHHIJIHI'IIllllllllll 31293 00913 4788 This is to certify that the thesis entitled The Relationship Between Risk Factors, Mothers' Level of Social Support and Stress Among Users of Respite Child Care presented by Maria Zeglen Townsend‘ has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. Child Development degree in Major professor Date M 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LEERARY Michigan Start. University i J z PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU In An Affirmative ActlorVEquel Opportunity Institution cMMan-M THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK FACTORS, MOTHERS’ LEVEL OF SOCIAL SUPPORT AND STRESS AMONG USERS OF RESPITE CHILD CARE by MARIA ZEGLEN TOWNSEND ATHESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OFARTS Department of Family and Child Ecology 1991 Abstract THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK FACTORS. MOTHERS’ LEVEL OF STRESS, AND SOCIAL SUPPORT AMONG USERS OF RESPITE CHILD CARE by Maria Zeglen Townsend There is relatively little empirical data on parents who use respite child care and the relationship between the use of these services and reduction in parental stress. A sample of two sub-groups; American and foreign mothers who used respite child care in a midwestern town was used to examine the relationships between identified risk factors and maternal levels of stress and social support. The majority of the mothers were married, high school graduates and living below the poverty threshold. The results include relationships between some of the identified risk factors and maternal levels of stress and social support. These relationships were often difi‘erent in magnitude and direction for the sub-groups. An interesting finding within the sub-groups was that the American mothers who used the respite child care service more Muently had higher levels of stress and the foreign mothers who use the service more frequently had lower levels of stress. DEDICATION I'd like to take this opportunity to dedicate this thesis to my mother, Joanne Zeglen. Though widowed in her forties with a teenage son and two schooled-aged daughters, she reared us in a home filled with love and taught each of us the importance of a college education. For your encouragement and all those Financial Aid Forms you complewd for me, thank you and I love you very much. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Dr. J eanne Brown for her guidance and friendship throughout my graduate career. You have been a wonderful friend and mentor. I would also like to thank the other members of my committee; Dr. Tom Luster for his comments and guidance, especially with chapter four, and Dr. Margaret Bubolz for her comments. Special thanks is extended to Judy Pfafi' for her patience and knowledge of statistics. I could not have done the data analysis as quickly as I did (and needed to) without your assistance. I would also like to thank a dear friend, Tracy, for helping me with the data collection on Thursday afternoons. I would like to thank the Family Growth Centers and the staff. Merry Stanford, for allowing me to conduct my study, along with Maureen and Delores for their assistance. Most especially, I would like to thank the mothers who participated in this study. Thank you for your time and honesty; without your help this thesis would not have been possible. You ARE the backbone of social research. I would also like to thank my husband, Ed, for the many meals you cooked, your editorial comments, your inspirational screen savers, and most especially your unwavering confidence in my ability to complete this thesis. It also deserves mentioning that you survived my roller coaster ride I of emotions throughout the writing of my thesis. I wish to thank all the members of my support network. I could not have survived all the “crises” of graduate school and research without you. I would like to thank my special FCE friends, Connie (for handling my ' long distance cries), along with Joy and Gloria. Thanks also to the “Chem iv Geeks” and their significant others; Kurt, Kris, Laura, Art, Mary, Allen, Jason, Sue, Berly, Gilbey, Gary, and all the rest, for all those great and sometimes humiliating times. TABLE OF CONIENTS P809 List of Tables ............................................................................ VIII List of Figures ........................................................................... IX Chapter 1. Identification of the Problem ...................................... 1 A . Introduction ................................................... 1 B. Purpose of Study .............................................. 2 C. Research Objectives ......................................... 2 D. Research Questions ......................................... 3 E. Rationale ........................................................ 3 Chapter 2. Review of Literature .................................................. 5 A . Introduction .................................................... 5 B. Parental Stress ................................................ 6 C. Risk Factors Associated with Parental Stress ................................................ 8 D. Parental Social Support ..................................... 17 E. Respite Child Care ........................................... 23 Chapter 3. Methodology ............................................................. so A. Research Objectives and Questions .................... 3) B. Conceptual and Operational Definitions .............. 3) C. Instrumentation ............................................. 33 1. Parenting Stress Index-Short Form .......... 33 2. Maternal Social Support Index ................. 35 3. Family Growth Center Questionnaire ........ 37 D. Sample Design ................................................. 38 E. Techniques of Data Collection ............................ $ F. Statistical Analysis ........................................... 41 vi G. Limitations of the Sampling Design and Data Collection Techniques ...................................... 42 Chapter 4. Results of Statistical Analysis ..................................... 44 A . Introduction .................................................... 44 B. Cleaning of the Data ......................................... 44 C. Reliability of the Instruments ............................ 45 D. Description of the Sample .................................. 47 E. Difi‘erences between the American and Foreign Mothers .......................................................... 49 F. Results of the Study ...................... . .................... 53 1. Question 1 .............................................. 53 2. Question 2 .............................................. 56 3. Question 3 .............................................. $ G. Chapter. Summary ........................................... 64 Chapter 5. Discussion and Implications ...................................... 67 A . Introduction .................................................... 67 B. Differences within the Sample ........................... 67 C. Discussion of the Research Objectives ................. a9 1. Research Objective 1 ................................ as 2. Research Objective 2 ................................ 72 3. Research Objective 3 ................................ 74 D. Summary ........................................................ 76 E. Future Research .............................................. 78 References .................................................................................. so Appendix A. Questionnaires ...................................................... $ Appendix B. Letters and Forms .................................................. Q LIST OF TABLES Pace Frequency distribution of income for the study sample ............ 48 Difi‘erences between the American and foreign mothers ......... 51 Correlations between the risk factors and the mothers’ PSI-SF scores by sub-groups and total sample ........................ 54 Correlations between the risk factors and the mothers’ MSSI scores by sub-groups and total sample .......................... 57 The relationship between the social support measures and level of stress for both sub-groups ......................................... 6) LIST OF FIGURES Flam A P386 41 Description of the study sample by ethnicity and race ........ 47 4-2 Scatterplot of the relationship between the MSSI scores and level of education for the foreign sub-group. .............. 58 1 Chapter 1 Identification of the Problem Introduction Respite care is a support service that ofi‘ers parents a break from the demands of caring for a dependent person. It is most often associated with care for handicapped or elderly pe0ple, and its primary intent is to provide support to the family, more specifically the primary caregiver (Salisbury & Intagliata, 1986). However, not all respite care programs target families with special needs children or elderly members; some programs target children who are at-risk of maltreatment. "This is a large population of the country's children, according to the Feb/March 1990 Children's Defense Fund Report fi-om Washington, D.C., every day in America 1,849 children i are abuse and neglected" (Peterson, 1990, September 9). Despite the fact that respite child care was developed to relieve parental stress and lessen the possibility of maltreatment, there have been few nonempirical research attempts to study the efi‘ects of respite child care. In fact, only three studies that have examined the efi'ects of respite care on parental stress are consistently cited in the literature (Rimmerman, 1989; Subramanian, 1986; Wikler, Hanusa, 8: Stoychefi', 1986). Consequently, funding for respite child care programs is difiicult to obtain because of the lack of evidence in the research literature that certifies its merit. Although the ideal study would be to determine if respite child care reduces maternal levels of stress through the use of pre- and post-test on mothers who use respite care, it was not possible at this time to conduct this type of comprehensive study. Because of restrictions imposed by the 2 Family Growth Centers (FGCs), the researcher chose a study that only required a one time participation of the mothers. Purposeof Study The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship of the identified risk factors on levels of social support and stress among those mothers who use respite child care. The measurement of the variables included two standardized questionnaires, one original questionnaire and the use of FGCs' records on the families. The three FGCs sites used for data collection are located in a midwestern town. They offer free, drop-in respite child care. Reseathhjecfives In order to achieve this purpose, more specific objectives were developed and were used as guides for this research: 1) To determine if the identified risk factors were related to the maternal level of stress in this sample; 2) To determine if the identified risk factors were related to the maternal level of social support in this sample; 3) To determine if the maternal levels of social support were related to the levels of maternal stress in this sample. Research W In order to accomplish the objectives of this study, several research question were addressed. 1. * What is the relationship between m and/or number of l ' identified risk factors the mothers had and their level of stress? These risk factors are: (a) low level of mcome, (b) low level of education, (c) high number of children, ((1) low number of adults residing at home, (e) high number of young children residing at home, (0 minority race status, (g) non-marital status. (h) teen parenthood status, (i) inadequate space in the home, and (j) low number of amenities in the home. .i r 5 ‘4'” P“ * 2’ What 18 the relationship between the type ‘and/drr‘n’umber of identified risk factors the mothers had and their level of social? support? These risk factors are: (a) low level of mcome, (b) low"M level of education, (c) high number of children, (d) low number of adults residing at home, (a) high number of young children residing at home, (i) minority race status, (g) non-marital status, and (h) teen parenthood status? 3. What is the relationship between the levels of social support and the levels of stress? Rationale This is an important area of research because there is little available, data on respite child care as a support for mothers (Joyce & Singer, 1983; Joyce, Singer, & Isralowitz, 1983; Wikler, Hanusa, & Stoychefi', 1986) and no data on respite child care as a moderating efi‘ect between identified risk factors and maternal levels of stress. By examining the moderating effects of respite child care on the maternal levels of stress, the FGCs may be able 4 to use the results of this study as evidence of the need for respite care applying for future grant proposals. This study will also add to the - literature in the area of respite child care. Since the sample was drawn utilizing a non-random sampling procedure, generalizability of the study will be limited to the parents using the three Family Growth Centers. 5 Chapter 2 Review of Literature Introduction The literature review is presented in four sections. In the first section, theoretical models of stress and how stress is related to the parental role will be presented to provide information needed to understand the relationship between the risk factors, maternal social support, and maternal stress. The second section discusses those studies focusing on risk factors associated with parental stress and support social. The third and fourth sections examine the moderating efi‘ects of parental social supports and use of respite child care services on the levels of parental stress. Most of the research in the area of respite child care focuses on physically and mentally handicapped children. The researcher believes that although there are specific problems faced by parents of handicapped children, there are also similarities between parents of handicapped children and parents who are at-risk for maltreatment of their children. Both sets of parents are experiencing stress in their parental role and both could benefit from time away from their children. Except for section four, respite child care services, the focus of this review of literature is on the identified variables' influence on high-risk populations. The rationale for this approach is that the study sample was drawn from mothers who use respite care, a program developed to serve a high risk population, i.e. mothers at-risk for child maltreatment. Parental Stress The ABCX Crisis Model was created by Hill, a sociologist, to explain why some families are able to c0pe with crises while others are not (McCubbin & Patterson, 1983). In this model, A is defined as the stressor event, B is defined as the family’s resources for meeting the stressor event, C is defined as the family’s perception of the stressor event, and X is defined as the crisis. Examples of stressors are transition to parenthood, teen pregnancy and parenthood, single-parenthood, and economic hardships related to job loss, divorce, and low levels of education. These stressors could originate outside the family, such as an economic depression, or inside the family, such as a divorce. How the family copes with the stressor depends on the resources the family has available to prevent the stressor from precipitating into a crisis event. Possible resources for a family include: familial cohesion, social support. collective goals of the family, agreement on role structure in the family, and members’ satisfaction with the family’s past experiences in meeting the members’ needs. Social support could come from within the family, the extended family, friends, agencies, or organizations. An example of an agency or organization could be the Family Growth Centers. In order for the family to decide to use their available resources, they must identify an event as a stressor event. The family’s perception of a stressor event is a reflection of the family's values and previous experiences in dealing with stressor events and crises (McCubbin & Patterson, 1983). According to the ABCX model, family stress is defined as an imbalance between the demands on the family, stressor events, and the family’s capability (resources) to overcome these events (McCubbin & Patterson, 1983). Whether the family experiences stress depends on many 7 things. These might include: the type of stressor event, the families resources, familial characteristics, and characteristics of individual family members. A black, single-parent mother may define an event as stressful, whereas a dual-career, white family may define the same event as not stressful. The ABCX Model focuses on what the family does when a stressful situation occurs and defines familial stress as an imbalance between. the family's demands and capabilities. The Parenting Stress Model focuses on sources of stress for parents that lead to dysfunctional parenting (Abidin, 1990). “It is posited that the total stress a parent experiences is a function of certain salient child characteristics, parent characteristics, and situations which are directly related to the role of being a parent’(Abidin, 1990, p. 2). The pertinent child characteristics include: the child’s ability to adapt to changes, the demands the child makes on the parent, being prone to excessive crying or withdrawal, hyperactivity, how close the child resembles the parents’ idealized child, and how much the child reinforces the parents’ in their role as parents. The parental characteristics include: depression, sense of competence in parenting role, and the investment of the parent in the parenting role. The parents’ relationship with each other, the amount of social support available to the parents, the parents’ health, and the restrictions placed on the parents due to the parenting role are all situational variables in this model. The Parenting Stress Index is based on the Parenting Stress Model. The short form of the Parenting Stress Index (PSI-SF) is a simplified version of the Parenting Stress Index. In the PSI-SF, the stress a parent is experiencing is a function of three things. They are: the distress parents feel in their role as parents, the parents’ perceptions of the child as not 8 meeting their expectations, and those characteristics of the child that make him/her either dificult or easy to parent. The amount of distress parents feel in their parenting role is related to the amount of available social support, restrictions placed on the parents due to their parenting roles, and conflict with spouse or significant other. These two models ofi'er some conceptual background on the variable of parental level of stress. The next three sections of this chapter will examine the relationship between risk factors (some of which could be - defined as stressor events), social support (an element in each of these two models), and parental level of stress. Risk Factors Associated with Parental Stress In this section, the relationship of certain risk factors found in high- risk families to the level of stresses experienced by these families is examined; These risk factors are income, education, number of children in the home, ages of children in the home, single parent status, minority status, parental age at birth of first child, amount of space in the home, and number of amenities in the home. Only those studies, that focus on the identified risk factors as they relate to the level of stress experienced by the parent are utilized to ensure that the information gained is closely related to the purpose of this study. Low level of income has been found to increase the level of stress experienced by parents (Andrews 8: Withey, 1979; Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988; McLanahan, 1983; Makosky, 1982; Pearlin & Johnson, 1977). The financial strain of raising children can be stressful for any parent regardless of Social Economic Status (SES) (Miller & Myers- 9 Wall, 1983). However, this stressor can be even more pronounced when the parent is in the low SES classification. In a national study examining the social indicators of well-being, people with lower SES expressed more negative evaluations of the following concerns: economic, family and marriage, health, and fun and enjoyment (Andrews & Withey, 1979). Also, this group experienced more stress in the areas of economic, family, and marriage concerns (Andrews & Withey, 1979). Among those people who reported low level of income, there was a disproportionate number of blacks and single-parents. The finding in the previous study that many of the people who are low SES were single-parents is not unusual. In fact, the average single-parent family income is half the average income for a two-parent family (Hogan, Bushler, & Robinson, 1983). This is the result of economic changes associated with divorce and lower levels of income for women in general. Therefore, it is not surprising that several studies have found that low level of income increases parental stress were focused on single-parents (Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988; McLanahan, 1983). In a study utilizing longitudinal data from the Panel Study of Income Dynamics, single-parent households were rated as experiencing more stress because of their increased likelihood of experiencing the following life strains: poor education, low income, and being black (McLanahan, 1983). It was also found that single-parent families had significantly higher scores on the Life Events Scale, with more disruptions in the areas of income, household composition, and work. By examining an all black, single-parent sample, researchers found that the mothers' demographic variables of income and education influenced their level of stress (Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988). 10 The sample included 70 functions] and 56 dysfunctional, low-income families. It was found that a mother's dissatisfaction with her income positively relawd to her summary stress score and both subscale stress scores. The subscales are the number of events requiring the most adjustment and the number of negative events reported by the families (rs=.19, .19, .22, respectively). In another study, 43 low-income mothers, who were not necessarily single-parents, were found to experience many stressful life events (Makosky, 1982). “These mothers reported from 5 to 33 events, with a median of 13.5 in one year. This compares to the average of 1 to 2 events per year found in most community surveys” (Makosky, 1982, p. 37). In a ranking of the areas of stressfulness completed by the mothers, concern or worry for money ranked the highest (60.1 of a possible 100) and was followed by the quality of living conditions, which was ranked 51.7. Based on data collected from 2,300 people in the Chicago area, the relationship between depression and both marital status and life strains were examined (Pearlin 8: Johnson, 1977). Though the study did not examine the relationship between economic strain and depression for single-parents, 38% of unmarried people in the sample were parents. It was found that the unmarried people in the sample experienced greater economic strain. In fact, 13% of the unmarried people, compared to 5% of the married people, reported experiencing severe economic strain. In this study, economic strain was defined as not having enough money to feed, clothe, or provide medical care for the family. Also, a relationship was found between the intensity of economic strain and level of depression for the general sample. Fifty-four percent of the 92 single people experiencing 11 severe economic strain scored the highest on the adopted measure of depre ssionr Low level of income is associated with increased levels of stress among parents. However, this relationship does not exist within a vacuum. Those parents that are experiencing stress due to economic strain are. experiencing stress from being a single-parent and/or being black (Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, 82. Lasely, 1988; McLanahan, 1983; Andrews & Withey, 1979). These two risk factors, marital status and race, are examined in more detail below. When comparing single-parents with married parents, single parents are found to be experiencing higher levels of stress (McLanahan, 1985; Weinraub & Wolf, 1983). A small sample of 28 mothers, halfof whom were single—parents, were interviewed to determine the efl'ects of stress on mother-child interaction (W einraub & Wolf, 1983). The single mothers reported an average of 2.6 more life changes than the married mothers. In another study involving over 8,000 parents, it was found that the well-being of single mothers declined by 11% over a one year period (McLanahan, 1985). This same study found that being a single mother was less stressful for black women than white women (McLanahan, 1985, p. 262). This last finding is contrary to other studies that examine the relationship between race and level of stress experienwd by the parents (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Conger et al., 1984; Pearlin & Johnson, 1977). Black families are subject to different types of stressors than white families because of racism (McAdoo, 1983). Blacks are overrepresented in the ranks of the unemployed and underrepresented on the enrollment lists of colleges and universities. These trends lead to the increased likelihood of blacks experiencing more stress due to lower levels of education and income. In 12 some studies, the most stressed families were black. This was determined by measures of well-being (Andrews & Withey, 1976) and the number of chronic stressors such as family income, number of parents, number of children, and education of mother (Conger et al., 1984). The interrelationship of race, number of children, and level of depression was examined in a previously mentioned study (Pearlin & Johnson, 1977). In this study, 2,300 people from the Chicago area were interviewed to determine the relationship between marital status, life strains, and depression. The results of the study indicated a positive relationship between the number of children in the home and level of depression of the parents. On the average, the married parents in the sample had more children than the single-parents. Among single-parents, black parents had a larger number of children than white parents and had higher levels of depression. The risk factors of single-parenthood and race were found to increase the level of stress for parents. Being black increases the chances of experiencing other risk factors such as being a single-parent, having a lower level of income, and having more children. Evidence of parenting stress is confirmed by the large decrease in the marital satisfaction and well-being of parents after the birth of the first child (Miller 8r Myers-Wall, 1983; Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). The relationship of marital satisfaction and children follows a U-shaped curve with the lowest point occurring when the oldest child is an adolescent and rises as the children leave home (Lamanna & Riedmann, 1988). The downward slope of this U-shsped curve may be related to the increase in the homework load and the decrease in the parent’s free time, which occurs with the addition of each new child to the family. 13 As mentioned in an earlier study, there is a positive relationship between the number of children in the home and the parents’ level of depression (Pearlin & J ohnson, 1977). This is true for both single-parents and married parents. However, single-parents experienced higher levels of depression as the number of children increases. Thirty-eight percent of the single-parents with three or more children scored the highest on the adapted depression scale compared to only 2.3% of the married parents. In a study conducted to determine the relationship between family ~ size, parent»child interaction, affect, and stress, it was found that as the number of children increased the parents experienced more stress in their role as parents (Nye, Carlson, & Garrett, 1970). The fathers in the study experienced an increase in strain as measured by disposition after the birth of the fourth child. The fathers were described as being more moody, having frequent displays of temper, and lacking cheerfulness. According to the authors, mothers experienced an increase in stress after the birth of the second child. The evidence for this trend was found in the decrease in the mother’s Satisfaction Index. There was no linear relationship between the number of children and parental stress. In fact, the authors feel that these findings are fragmentary and only suggestive of a relationship (Nye, Carlson, & Garrett, 1970). Instead, they suggest that the increase in parental stress may be partially caused by the decrease in family income level, which is a direct result of the increase in the number of children. In a study conducted by McLanahan (1985), the number of children had an inverse efi'ect on the changes in well-being of the parent. Parents with a large number of children experienced a decrease in their scores of well-being from Time 1 (1968), to Time 2 (1971). It was found that single mothers were less afi‘ected by the number of children than married fathers. 14 This result is similar to the finding in the Nye, Carlson, and Garrett study (1970). That is, the decrease in the fathers’ disposition was greater than the - decrease in either the" mothers’ disposition or Satisfaction Index scores. Not only does the number of children affect the levels of stress experienced by the parents, but the ages of the children also afi'ect the level of stress. For example, physical stressors of parenting are greater on the parents of young children compared to the parents of adolescents (Miller 8: Myers-Walls, 1983). This is because of the greater dependence of the younger child on the parent to meet all of his/her needs. Some studies do not directly examine the relationship between ages of children and parental levels of stress. Instead, they define this risk factor as a stressor (McLanahan, 1983; Pittman, Wright, & Lloyd, 1989). In a study utilizing data from the General Social Surveys, an inverse relationship was found between the ages of children and the measures of well-being for the adults (Glenn & Weaver, 1979). The presence of children in the home was found to have a negative efi’ect on the parents’ well-being as measured by the global happiness question. This question asks the respondent to rate how they think things are going these days. Having children between the ages of birth through five years was negatively related to the fathers’ well-being but not for mothers. Mothers’ well-being was negatively related to having children who were between the ages of six to twelve years. Both fathers’ and mothers’ well-being was negatively related to having adolescent children. More significant findings could have been found if a less global and more specific measure of well-being was utilized. A relationship between age of children, marital status, and depression was found in a study conducted by Pearlin and Johnson (1977). Single-parents of children five years and younger had higher levels of l5 depression than those parents with children who are six years or older. “The gamma coeficient for this association among parent whose youngest child is five years or less is .48; for those whose youngest is from 6 to 17, the coeficient is .39; and it declines further to .13 among parents having a youngest child over 18" (Pearlin & Johnson, 1977, p. 712). Just as the age of the children can afi‘ect the parents’ level of stress, so can the age ofthe parent at the birth ofthe first child. In a study of 107 families drawn from three samples in two difl‘erent states, the relationship between the mother’s age and maternal behavior was examined (Conger et al., 1984). The three samples varied in levels of risk, from high risk to middle class families. In one of the two models-the chronic stress model beingexsminedinthestudy, theauthors defineageatfirstbirthasa stressor. This is consistent with another study that defined age at first birth as a chronic life strain (McLanahan, 1983). This definition is logical because teen parenting is associated with chronic environmental and psychological stressors, such as low levels of education and income, and single-parenthood. Testing of the second model (life experiences model), revealed a relationship between the chronic-stress measure and age at birth of first child (Conger et al., 1984). The chronic-stress was a combination of four demographic variables: family income, number of parents, number of children, and education of mother. This measure was negatively related to the age at first birth for all three samples, but significant for only two samples. It was not significant for the highest-risk sample. The authors suggested that this may be a result of the sample’s homogeneity. Age at first birth influences the levels of stress experienced by the parents. This risk factor is often accompanied by the risk factors of low 16 income, single-parenthood, and low education. When a person becomes a parent at a young age, their education is often interrupted in order to assume this- new parental role and to handle the added responsibilities. Social scientists recognize the efi‘ect of low levels of education on the lives of people and often define low level of education as a stressor in research studies (Conger et al., 1984; McLanahan, 1983; Zuravin, 1988). f In the study conducted by Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, and Lasley (1988), it was found that the “nonclinic single-parents reported significantly more positive educational changes than did the clinic single-parents” (p. 111). Not only did the nonclinic single-parents report significantly more positive educational changes, but they also reported fewer stressful life events. Thus, the clinic families experienced more negative educational changes and more stressful life events. Several of the studies listed the family’s living conditions as being stressful to the parents (Makosky, 1982; McAdoo, 1983). McAdoo stated that an indirect result of racism’s denial of economic opportunities is inadequate housing. Many blacks live in houses or apartments that are too small and are located in high-crime neighborhoods. In Makesst (1982) study of 43, low-income mothers, she found that only 14% of the sample lived in the more spacious single-family dwellings. * Though not researched in the articles dealing with parental stress, lack of space and lack of amenities in the home can be stressful to the parent. A parent who has no phone may worry about what to do if her child needs immediate medical help or she may be socially isolated. The lack of the basic amenities, such as running water and electricity can increase the work load of a parent and her level of stress. 17 The literature has shown a relationship between parental level of stress and the following identified risk factors: income, single-parenthood, race, number of children, ages of children, age at first birth, and education (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Conger et al., 1984; Glenn & Weaver, 1979; Lindblad—Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988; Makosky, 1982; McAdoo, 1983; McLanahan, 1983 and 1985; Nye, Carlson, and Garrett, 1970; Pearlin and Johnson, 1977; Weinraub 8: Wolf, 1983). Some of the evidence ofl‘ered here to support this relationship is weak, especially for the risk factors of ages and number of children, education, and age at birth of first child (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Conger et al., 1984; Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988; Nye, Carlson, & Garrett, 1970). However, the fact that many of these risk factors are cited as stressors in the research suggests a common definition of these risk factors as stressors for parents (McLanahan, 1983; Pittman, Wright, & Lloyd, 1989; Zuravin, 1988). Parental Social firppm't In this section, studies examining the moderating efi‘ect of social support on parental levels of stress are summarized. Social support is one of the four characteristics of social networks; the other three are structure, interaction, and quality. ”Its function is defined as the behavior of 1" providing information and feedback, comfort and cheer, material aid, advice, help in constructive problem solving, and so forth” (Heller & Swindle, 1983, p 92). « According to the Buffering Hypothesis, social support protects people against the efi‘ects of stressful life events on the person’s well-being (Wilcox, 1981). However, this hypothesis found limited support in a study conducted by Wilcox (1981) involving 500 people from a large Southwestern city. 18 Multiple regression analysis of the data revealed that social support, as measured by the the types of support received from the social network, explained only 12% of the variance of the interaction between Life Event scores and the Langner Symptom Checklist. This finding suggests that social support is somewhat important as a bufl'er for stressful life events, but it does not explain the whole relationship. This could be a result of this hypothesis’ failure to account for the individual difl‘erences in the sample’s networks and utilization of the support ofi'ered (Heller & Swindle, 1983). In order to examine the relationship between social support and parental stress as reported in the literature, the following studies will be grouped according to similarities in the characteristics of the sample. This will allow for the examination of differences according to groups of mothers. Those studies whose sample are low-income women will be examined first, followed by studies whose focus are single-parents, and finally, the studies examining teen parents. The first study examined the mediating efi'ects of social support on low income women and found that the level of support ofi‘ered by one’s spouse or significant other was positively related to the mother’s level of stress (Zur-Szpiro & Longfellow, 1982). Mothers’ parenting stress scores were correlated with the type of suppert the husbands or significant others provided. The types of support that were significantly correlated were child care support, help with household tasks, and emotional support. These three types of support were also significantly correlated to the mothers’ depression scores. Utilizing data from the same study, Belle (1982) found that women with large social networks did not experience less depressive symptoms or have lower stress scores. They also found no relation to the amount of 19 contact the mothers had with a member of their social networks or their proximity to them. Instead, these were significantly related to the mothers ' increased worry level about her friends and relatives. The author suggests that for low income mothers, the support they receive from their network members is both a relief and a burden. This “double-edged sword” may be the reason for the non-significant findings between the correlation of social _ support from family and friends with levels of stress. The ”double-edged sword” is not just a characteristic of this one study sample. In a paper examining the role of family social networks in moderating the effects of familial stress, Unger and Powell (1980) cited several studies that have found both positive and negative effects of social networks on familial stress. The reason for this dichotomy is the reciprocal nature of support among network members. Though the members received needed support, they are also responsible for providing support to other members of the network. In a study conducted by Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, and Lasely (1988 ), it was found that not all support or contact with family members have a positive influence on the mothers’ level of stress. This study was mentioned in the first section of this review. The sample included 126 black, low- income, single-parents. The study found that the family stress score was higher if the single-parent mother had more contacts with relatives and if her support network members were younger (Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988). In contrast to the findings supporting the “doubleoedged sword” characteristic of social networksYa/study of low-income, single-parents _ found that the support of friends and relatives was significantly related to the measures of well-being (Gladow & Ray, 1986). The study measured the /. / 2) support received from friends, relatives, neighbors, and the community for sample of 63, single-parent mothers. Total support and its two' subscales of friend and relative support were negatively related to the severity of the problems faced by these single-parent mothers‘/7A stepwise regression revealed that the total support score explained/42% of the variance of total problems, the study measure of the everyday stresses experienced by the mothers. One possible reason for this last study’s contrary findings is that the support received fi'om those peeple who are similar to the receiver is more efl’ective (Thoits, 1986). People who are similar have experienced similar stressors and are able to ofl‘er more efi‘ective support in the form of problem- solving advice and emotional support. This support can be ofi'ered by family, friends, and co-workers. r: " In another study of low-income, single-parent mothers, the social « support provided by friends and co-workers was found to contribute to the X" mothers’ level of well-being (D'Ercole, 1988). Eighty-three single mothers 1‘ from New York city were administered objective measures of level of stregg, strain. and well-being. Social support of friends and co-workers was negatively related to a global measure of psychological f symptoms experienced by the mothers. Level of social support was found to bufi‘er mothers against the stress of low SES. Those mothers with high levels of = support fi'om fiiends and co-workers were lggsfllikely to experience ”financialstr-ain due to the stressor of low-income. ‘7 ,, From the above studies of single-parents, it was found that those women who received support from friends, family, and co-workers scored higher on measures of well-being and lower on measures of stress. Even though single-parent mothers seem to benefit from the support they receive 21 from their social networks, they are less likely to receive this support. This is a result of their smaller social networks. Two studies cited in section one found that single-parent families had smaller social networks (McLanahan, 1983; Weinraub & Wolf, 1983). When compared to married families, female-headed families were less likely to know their neighbors and lived farther away from relatives (McLanahan, 1983). In addition, they were less likely to belong to social organizations and to receive free help with household responsibilities. In the other study, 14 single mothers experienced more stressful life events, were more socially isolated, and had less support than the married mothers (Weinraub & Wolf, 1983). Frequency of social contacts were less for single-parent mothers compared to the married mother. Single mothers also reported receiving less emotional support and tended to rate their family and friends as less supportive. However, single mothers did receive more support than married mothers in area of practical help with child care. Not only do older single-parents have smaller networks, but so do teen mothers. Teenagers who become mothers are out of step with the rest of their peers. They are experiencing parental stressors that most of their peers and friends are not experiencing. Their network members are not very characteristically similar to them, and they may even lose network members due to these dissimilarities. However, these teens may now find themselves more similar to their family members. This is because some of the teens that find themselves pregnant are themselves children of teen parents (Hayes, 1987). Therefore, it is not surprising to find that the teen mothers received more effective support from family members than from their peers. 22 In a study of teen mothers, a correlation between the level of overall satisfaction and family support between the first and second measurement was found (Unger & Wandersman, 1988). The majority of the mothers were black (89%), single (93%), and lived at home with one or two of their parents (74%). The teens were interviewed during their second trimester and again when their child was eight months old. Family support, but not partner support, was significantly related to fewer concerns over financial and residential issues at both the prenatal and second interviews. In another study, it was found that the greatest amount (30.75%) of variance in the teen mothers’ emotional stress score was accounted for by her total support scale (Colletta & Gregg, 1981). The sample consisted of three groups of pregnant teens: 25 who were in a Baltimore City Public Schools Infant-Parent program, 25 mothers who were in school, and 14 mothers who had dropped out of school. The people who ofi‘ered the social support were not defined. Another finding of this study was that the level of emotional stress experienced by the adolescent mother was negatively related to level of social support for all three groups. In a later reporting of the data from the same study, a significant relationship between total support and emotional stress experienced by adolescent mothers was found for the program and dropout mothers . (Colletta & Lee, 1983). For drop-out mothers, the specific types of support that were negatively related to overall emotional stress were living arrangements, housework, and support during illness. For the program mothers, care and living arrangements, peer group support, support during illness, child care information, and support from individuals were all significantly related to their overall emotional distress. The findings support the hypothesis that adolescent mothers are not a homogeneous 23 group of mothers, but rather have different levels of support available to them. The articles reviewed in this section suggest that there are difi'erences among the groups of mothers in the moderating effect of social support on parental stress. Some of the studies provide support for the “doubleedged sword” of social support received from one’s social network with the mothers experiencing both positive and negative efi'ects (Belle, 1982; Lindblad-Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988). However, other studies found support for the idea that increased efi'ectiveness of social support is because of the increased similarity of social network members (D’Ercole, 1988; Gladow & Ray, 1986; Zur-Szpiro & Longfellow, 1982). There were differences among the teen mothers in terms of types of support received (Colletta & Lee, 1983) and who gave the support (Unger 8r Wandersman, 1988). Respite Child Care In this section, the moderating effects of respite child care on parental level of stress are examined. “Respite child care is child care 1 provided on a short-term basis for families who are experiencing stress due to medical or emotional problems, or who are going through dificult transitions and need relief” (Weinstein & Siegel, 1989, p 4). These services can be offered for several hours or for a few weeks in the home of the family or at a center. Respite care ofi'ered at the Wisconsin Respite Center has been shown to reduce parental levels of stress (Subramanian, 1985). This center serves a high-risk population who are at-risk for child abuse. A pro-and post-test 24 methodology was utilized with a sample of thirty-six parents who had used the center over a six month period. The study found that the overall stress . level of the mothers decreased from Time I to Time II. The biggest decrease was found in the subscale of Tension/Anxiety. All the other subscales, Depression/Dejection, Anger/Hostility, and Confirsion/Bewilderment, showed significant decreases. The only exception was for Fatigue/Inertia, which increased. The increase in this scale could be a result of the mothers accomplishing necessary tasks while their child was in respite child care. / A study by Subramsnian (1985) was the only empirical study conducted on a respite child care center whose main purpose is preventing child abuse of non-handicapped children. What is found in the literature are summaries of the services ofi‘ered by respite child care programs that serve a non-handicapped child population, but with no empirical data on program outcomes (Allison, Kilgallon, & Reilly, 1988; Franz, 1980, J anuary/February; Hansenfeld, Murphy, & Olson, 1981; O’Connor, Davis, & Sahlien, 1984). The article by Franz (1980, January/February), explains in more detail the Wisconsin Respite Center that was used as a data collection site in Subramsnian’s (1985) article. The goals, services, and problems of the center are discussed in detail and helpful hints are given for those people who are considering developing a similar program. Two respite care centers that serve children 5 years and younger were discussed in two articles (Allison, Kilgallon, & Reilly, 1988; O’Connor, Davis, 8: Sahlien, 1984). Parents Place Incorporated’s main goal is to reduce parental stress caused by the demands of young children (O’Connor, Davis, & Sahlien, 1984). Anecdotal examples were employed to explain the techniques used by the stafi‘ of social workers and early % childhood professionals to reach the goals of the program. Children’s Cottage is a 24 hour crisis nursery utilized by battered women who have left their homes (Allison, Kilgallon, & Reilly, 1988). The children are cared for by trained stafl', while the mothers deal with such crises as finding suitable housing or a job. Another article explains the programs of three respite care centers in Michigan (Hansenfeld, Murphy, 8: Olson, 1981). These centers are the Family Growth Center, Pooh’s Place and the Washtenaw County Centers. The article discusses the child care program, level of parental involvement, funding, and agency support for all three centers. Though no empirical data were collected, the article does state that parental involvement in the programs was varied. Those articles that provide basic information about existing respite care centers do so because the authors feel there is a need for this type of programing. The day-care system in the United States does not provide child care services to the population who needs them most, the low-income, at-risk mothers (Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). What is needed are respite child care centers that offer free, quality child-care. More empirical studies involving these centers are needed to show that these centers are making a difl'erence. For more information on the moderating efi‘ect of respite child care on parental levels of stress, the only literature available is in the area of respite care for handicapped children. The literature on respite care for handicapped children is more extensive than the literature on respite child care for non-handicapped children. The respite care programs for both populations have similar goals. Two main goals of respite care for handicapped children are to reduce emotional costs to the family and to help prevent burnout that might % precipitate neglect, family disruptions, or institutional placement (Butler & Friensen, 1988). These goals are similar to the goals of respite child care for parents at-risk for abusing their children. Therefore, the findings of studies utilizing parents of handicapped children could provide additional insight into the moderating effects of respite child care on parental levels of stress. Mothers who used respite care experienced a decrease in their levels of stress (Rimmerman, 1989). A pre- and post-test methodology was utilized to compare 32 mothers who used home-based, respite care to 25 mothers who were non-users. The short form of the Questionnaire on Resources and Stress was used to measure the maternal levels of stress. It was found that the users of respite child care experienced a significant reduction in their scores from Time I to Time II. The study found changes over time in the level of stress and coping resources for both groups. These changes could have been a result of the mothers dealing with the handicap and learning new ways to cope. However, only these mothers who used the home-based, respite care experienced a significant decrease in level of stress. In another study on respite care, families experienced a reduction in their levels of stress and burdens of care (Wikler, Hanusa & Stoychefi', 1986). Two projects were conducted in which home-based respite care was provided by college students for parents of children with handicaps. A pre— and post-test methodology was utilized. The parents level of stress was significantly reduced from Time I to Time II. Many of the parents also experienced an increase in social relations. This increase was probably caused by the increase in the parents’ free time, which they used to cultivate new friendships. 27 This increase in the size of the parents’ social network is probably not unusual. Because of the increased amount of care necessary for a handicapped child, the parents often have smaller networks and higher levels of stress compared to parents of non-handicapped children (Salisbury, 1986). The decrease in the levels of stress may also be a result of the increase in the parents’ social networks. In 1983, it was found that the parents who used respite care experienced emotional relief and an increase in their social activities (Joyce . & Singer, 1983; Joyce, Singer, & Isralowitz, 1983). Twenty-four families completed questionnaires designed to determine their perceptions of respite care. The families indicated that the service was helpful in relieving the emotional and physical strain associated with caring for a special needs child. They also reported an increase in their social activities, which is a direct result of the fi'ee time they had when the child was in respite care. The literature in this area, though extensive, faces the problem of little methodological sophistication and small sample sizes (Intagliata, 1986). Few empirical studies have been conducted and the results that are reported are'about the satisfaction levels of parents regarding the program (Fagan & Gurry, 1981: Frisbie & Slater, 1979; Stenson, 1989). The studies that report parents’ satisfaction with respite care are discussed below. Though these studies ofi‘er little pertinent information, the fact that parents are satisfied with the services suggests a decrease in their level of parental stress related to child care. In a study conducted by Frisbie and Slater (1979), the majority of the parents reported being very satisfied with the respite care services. The study consisted of only 35 parents which represented a 20% return rate. Sixty-one percent of the parents reported being very satisfied with the 8 emergency respite care service. The study also found that the parents were very satisfied with the other respite care services: Family Friend (81%), Foster (71%), and Home-Based (57%). Only 12% of the parents reported that they were very dissatisfied with the Foster Respite Care. Though this study offers some information, the small sample size, low response rate, and lack of data on other measures decreases the importance of the findings. 'le0 other studies found the parents to be satisfied with respite care services (Fagan & Gurry, 1981; Stenson, 1989). In a pilot study, ten parents who utilized home-based respite care offered by Temporary Care Services, Inc. found the service to be very satisfactory (Fagan & Gurry, 1981). The parents mentioned that the program’s flexible hours and quality workers were the most appreciated elements of the program. Saturday Afternoon Free is a respite care program that is offered on weekends for families with moderate to severely handicapped children (Stenson, 1989). The program serves 15 children each semester. An evaluation conducted over two semesters polled 30 parents. On a scale of one to five with five being the highest, all of the parents rated the program a five. What the scale was measuring was not mentioned in the artic1e. Several studies have found that respite child care reduces parental levels of stress (Wikler, Hanusa & Stoychefi', 1986; Joyce & Singer, 1983; Joyce, Singer, 8; Isralowitz, 1983; Rimmerman, 1989; Subramsnian, 1985). Many of the studies sufi'ered from poor methodology, small sample sizes, and lack of additional variables. Three of the studies claimed respite child care was efl‘ective because the parents expressed satisfaction in the services (Fagan & Gurry, 1981; Frisbie & Slater, 1979; Stenson, 1989). There is a need for more empirical studies that examine the outcomes of respite child care. Of interest to this researcher was the role of 3 respite care as a moderator of the relationship between the risk factors identified in section one and the mothers’ level of stress. In this study, the _ usage of respite child care was defined as one measure of social support. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the influence of the identified risk factors on the maternal levels of stress and social support among those mothers who use respite child care. 1!) Chapter 3 Methodology Research Otrjectives and Questions The research objectives and questions were stated in Chapter one . ' pages 2 and 3. These objectives and questions define the focus of the study: Conceptual and W Definitions In this section, the major variables of the study will be defined. The major variables are the mother's level of stress and social support and the identified risk factors. The format of this section will be: conceptual definition first, followed by the operational definition for both the dependent and independent variables. We is the level of stress experienced by the mothers as a result of their children's behavioral characteristics, parental distress, and stresses derived fi-om the interaction between parent and child (Touliatos, Perlmutter, & Straus, 1990). Operationally, this was measured using the Parenting Stress Index Short Form (PSI-SF) (Abidin, 1990) a questionnaire in which the . mother responded to 36 items on a five-point Likert scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree (Appendix A, pages 95 to 97, items 1- 36). The short form includes three subscales: Parental Distress (PD), Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction (P-CDI), and the Dificult Child (DC). The mothers recorded their responses directly on the questionnaire. The researcher reverse scored all of the responses to obtain subscale scores and then added all three subscale scores to obtain the Total Stress Score. W is the amount of instrumsntal and functional support the mothers receive fi-om their social network and formal social agencies. Operationally, this was measmd by using 31 the Maternal Social Support Index (MSSI) (Pascoe, Loda, Jefi'ries, & Earp, 1981) and a rating of the mothers use of the respite child care services at the Family Growth Centers (FGCs). The mothers responded to the 21-item MSSI questionnaire by checking the appropriate column or circling the appropriate answer (Appendix A, pages 91 and 93, items 1-21). The index has two identified clusters of items; Child Care Tasks (items 3, 8, and 9) and Non-Child Care Tasks (items 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, and 10). A score was obtained by adding item weights and summing the results to obtain a total score. The maximum score for the MSSI is 39. A high score indicates higher support from the social network. The literature does not suggest that a score be used as a cut-ofi‘ point between low and high social support scores. A second variable of low and high maternal social support was created by placing the scores into two categories: 1) low maternal social support and 2) high maternal social support. High support was defined as having a score above the mean for this sample. The mean MSSI score for the sample was 21.6. This follows the procedure used by Pascoe and Earp (1984) in their study to determine the efi‘ect of mothers’ social support and life changes on the the home environment of their children. A measure of the maternal social support received fiom the FGCs was determined by both the length of time and the average monthly usage of the respite child care services. The mothers were asked to indicate the month and the year they began using the respite care services and approximately how many times a month they used these services (Appendix A, page 87, items 5 and 6). Winning is the total take home income of the family. It was determined from the files at the Family Growth Centers. It was coded by placing them into the following categories based on family size: 0) above poverty threshold, 1) poverty threshold, 2) 20% below poverty threshold, 3) 40% below poverty threshold, 4) 60% below poverty threshold, and 5) 80% below poverty threshold. The poverty threshold by family size is based on the results of the 1990 census and is currently being used by the Michigan State Department of Social Services to determine eligibility for services (Tellar, 1991). 32 Won is the total number of years completed in school by the mother. The mothers were asked to list the last year of school they completed (Appendix A, page 89, item 14). It was scored by placing the number into the appropriate category: 1) 8, 2) 9, 3) 10, 4) 11, 5) 12, 6) 13, 7) 14, 8) 15, 9) 16, 10) 17 or more. ThaNumbsLnflAdulmLinnainflmz The mothers were asked to indicate the first names, ages, and relationship for all the people living in their home. Those responses that fit the criterion of being over the age of 18 years old were utilized. The number of adults was used as the code for the response (Appendix A, page 90, item 17). Wm: The mothers were asked to indicate the first names, ages, and relationship for all the people living in the home (Appendix A, page 90, item 17). These responses that fit the criterion of being under the age of 18 were utilized. The number of children was used as the code for the response. WWW: The mothers were asked to indicate the age of each child (Appendix A, page 90, item 17). The responses were then placed into the following categories: 1) Birth to 5 years, 2) 6 to 11 years, and 3) 12 to 17 years. The number assigned to each response was used as the code for the response. Wm: Each subjects’ race/ethnicity was determined from the files at the Family Growth Centers. It was coded by placing them into the following categories: 1) Caucasian, 2) Asian, 3) Black, 4) Hispanic, 5) Middle—Eastern. A second variable of minority status was created by placing the scores into two categories: 1) Caucasian and 2) minority. This second category, minority includes the race categories of 2 (Asian) through 5 ( Middle Eastern). We: The mothers were asked to indicate their marital status (Appendix A, page 89, item 13). The response categories were: 1) Never married, 2) Married, 3) Separated, 4) Divorced, 33 5) Widowed. A second variable was created by placing the scores into two categories: 0) Married and 1) Single. We: Teen Parent status was determined from the files at the Family Growth Centers. A teen parent status was assigned to those mothers who were under the age of twenty when they gave birth to their first child. It was coded by placing them into the following categories: 1) under the age of 20 years and 2) 20 years or older. W: The mothers were asked to rank the amount of space they have in their home on a 7-point Likert Scale ranging fi-om not enough room (one) to more than enough room (seven) (Appendix A, page 93, items 22). The number assigned to each response was used as the code for the response. Wanna: The mothers were asked to circle all of the following amenities they have in their home (Appendix A. page 94, items 23): 1) Running water, 2) Electricity, 3) Heat, 4) Telephone, 5) Refiigerator, 6) Stove/oven, 7) Radio, 8) Television, 9) Washer, and 10) Dryer. The total number of amenities circled was used as the code for the total number of amenities in the home. In order to add the ownership of a car to the total number of amenities, a one was added to the total number of amenities if the mother choose 2, 3, or 4 as her response for item ten in the MSSI (Appendix A, page 91). Item ten reads: Who takes care of car problems on short notice (if appropriate)? Answer one is no car. Instrumentation EarsnfinLStresLIndexzfihomEom One standardized instrument, the Parenting Stress Index Short Form (PSI-SF) was used in this study to measure the variable of maternal 34 level of stress. The PSI-SF is based on the 101-item Parenting Stress Index (PSI) developed by Richard R. Abidin in 1976. The PSI measures the stress _ experienced by parents of children ten years of age and younger. Originally, a letter was sent asking for the PSI and manual. However, the PSI-SF was chosen because of pressures from the Family Growth Center for a shorter questionnaire and a telephone conversation with Dr. Abidin about the present research. It was during this conversation that Dr. Abidin suggested that the short form of the PSI would work better with this particular sample population of high-risk, possibly low-education mothers because it would ofi'er the needed information to answer the proposed research questions in a shorter questionnaire (Abidin, personal communication, 11/9/90). Since the PSI-SF was only recently developed (1990), independent data on reliability and validity for this scale was unavailable. However, the items that comprise the 36-item, short form are original items from the 101- item PSI. The concurrent validity between the two scales is shown in the high correlation of .94 for the total stress scores of the PSI and the PSI-SF (Abidin, 1990). Two of the three subscale scores of the PSLSF are highly correlated with the domain scores of the PSI subscales. The Parental Distress and Dificult Child Subscales of the PSI-SF have Pearson correlations of .92 and .87 with the analogous areas of the PSI (Abidin, 1990). The third subscale of the PSI-SF, the Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction subscale, has a lower correlation with both the Parent Domain (r: .50) and the Child Domain (rs .73) on the PSI (Abidin. 1990). W The maternal level of social support was assessed by using the Maternal Social Support Index (MSSI). (Pascoe, Leda, J efi'ries, & Earp, 1981). It was selemd because it specifically measures subjective and objective aspects of maternal support in everyday activities. This is accomplished through a 21-item questionnaire assessing seven areas: 1) help with daily tasks, 2) satisfaction with visits fi'om kin or relatives, 3) help with crisis, 4) emergency child care, 5) satisfaction with communication fi'om a male support figure, 6) satisfaction with communication fi-om another adult, and 7) community involvement (Pascoe et al., 1987). There are several studies that determine the validity and reliability of the MSSI with difi'erent types of samples. The MSSI has been used to explain the variance measured by the Csldwell's Inventory of Home Stimulation (IHS) in a study involving parents of infants who had been in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit in North Carolina (Pascoe, Loda, Jefi‘ries, & Earp, 1981). The sample was predominantly rural and working class; 59% of the mothers were black and 41% were white. The average level of education for the sample was tenth grade. The MSSI was found to explain between 12% and 22% of the variance on three of the IHS subscales. In the same study, it was found that these mothers who perceived more social support scored higher on the IHS scale regardless of the amount of life change experienced by them as assessed by the Rahe’s Schedule of Recent Life Events (Pascoe & Earp, 1984). In another study involving 219 predominantly white women, the MSSI was slightly correlamd with the quality of child rearing by the mother when the infant was four months of age (rs .30) and the mother's score on the Cranley's Maternal/Fetal Attachment Scale (r- .30) (French, 1990). 3 MSSI scores were found to be predictive of child maltreatment and low birth weight infants (Pascoe et al., 1987; Pascoe, Walsh-Clifi'ord, & Earp, 1982). In a study to determine construct validity of the MSSI, the scores for two groups of mothers, protective service referred and non- protective service referred, were compared. The means for the protective- service mothers was 7.4 and for the non-protective-service mothers the mean was 10.5 (Pascoe, Walsh-Clifi'ord, 8: Earp, 1982). The difi‘erence between the means was significant. In another study involving 222 mothers fi-om a depressed area in Toledo, Ohio, there was a significant association between certain items on the MSSI and low birth weight (Pascoe et al., 1987). It was found that these mothers who did not receive help with grocery shopping, fixing things, and paying bills were more likely to give birth to a low birth weight infant. The MSSI was found to have acceptable test-retest reliability in two studies. In the first study, one hundred mothers in a mdiatric clinic subgroup were retested six to eight weeks after the initial interview; the _ test-retest reliability coeficient was .72 (Pascoe et al., 1988). Another study examining the social support of primiparous mothers found a correlation of .75 for retest scores after a nine month period (Pascoe & French, 1990). This same study found an inverse relationship between the scores on the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale and MSSI at both six weeks and nine months postpartum. The internal consistency of the two clusters, Child Care Tasks and Non-Child Care Tasks, was determined in a study with 488 mothers fi-orn three clinics: prenatal, pediatric, and psychology (Pascoe et al., 1988). A Cronbach’s Alpha was calculated for all three subgroups and a range of .58 to .72 was found for the Child Care Cluster and .49 to .78 for the Non-Child 37 Care Cluster. The authors suggest that the low coeflicients are probably due to the small number of items in each cluster. E'lGIlGIQI" The Family Growth Center Questionnaire is a questionnaire created by the researcher to assess the mothers’ usage of the respite child care services and to obtain additional demographic information not available in the mothers’ files at the FGCs (Appendix A, pp 86-90). The seventeen-item - questionnaire is divided into two parts. The first part consists of eleven items that ask the mothers how they learned about the FGCs and its services, when they began to use the respite child care, what they do when their children are at the FGCs, and if they have ever used any of the other services ofi'ered by the FGCs. The purpose of these eleven questions is to obtain information for the FGCs and to ensure eligibility of the mothers by determining that they are not a member of either the FGCs’ support groups or parent classes. The second part of the questionnaire obtains needed demographic information on the mothers and members of their household that cannot be found in the FGCs files. This part consists of six items. During the pretest, the mothers had no trouble completing this . questionnaire. After the pretest was” conducted, an additional question was added to bring the total items to seventeen. This item asks the mothers how they learned about the services at the FGCs. The services were listed in column A and five possible sources including “other” were liswd under column B. The mothers were asked to match the services in column A to the sources in column B. Although the structure of matching was chosen to simplify the answering of the item for the mothers, it was often incorrectly completed by the mothers during the study. Many of the 38 mothers underlined services in column A and sources in column B without drawing a line to connect the two. This item was added because of a request by the stafi‘ at the FGCs; therefore, incorrect or incomplete responses will not sfl'ect the ability to answer the proposed research questions. SampleDesisn A non-random sampling procedure was used in this study. The sample for this study was drawn from those mothers who used only the respite child care service at three Family Growth Centers in a midwestem town fi-om February 11, 1991 to May 10, 1991. At first, one of the centers was not included in the sample frame because of the large number of foreign parents fi'om a nearby university who use the facility. It was felt that the inclusion of the foreign mothers, who have difl'erent levels of assimilation into our culture, could affect the results of the study. However, due to the small number of mothers using the other two centers, the third center was added. Separate analyses were condumd with the data fiom the foreign mothers to determine if they were significantly difi'erent fi'om the other mothers on the variables of interest. All mothers who were using the respite child care at the three centers fi'om February 11, 1991 to May 10, 1991 were asked by the researcher to participate in the study. A list of eligible mothers was compiled fi-om the files at the three FGCs. The criteria used to select the eligible mothers were: (1) the mothers were using only the respite child care services and (2) were not presently members of any of the support groups or attending parent education classes offered at the FGCs. Because this sample fi'ame consisted of approximately 90 mothers, every efi‘ort was made by the researcher to contact all eligible mothers to ask them to participate. Due to 19 the non—random sampling design of the study, the generalizability of the L results of this study will be limited to the mothers who use respite care at the three Family Growth Centers. Techniques of Data Collection A meeting between the researcher, the director, and the two receptionists at the Family Growth Centers was held prior to data collection to inform the stafi' about the study and provide instruction on recruiting mothers for the study. At this meeting, a time and place for a pro-test of the questionnaires was determined. A pro-test was conducted to determine any possible problems with the selected instruments and the researchers’ original questionnaire. One of the existing support groups, with no mothers who were eligible for this study, was chosen to complete the questionnaires and to answer questions about the ease of completing and understanding the questionnaires (Appendix B, pp 104-105). Data were collected on the mothers fi-om February 11, 1991 until May 10, 1991. Posters advertising the study were displayed at the centers to ensure that every efi'ort was made to reach eligible mothers (Appendix B, p 106). The receptionists asked the mothers if they would like to participate in a research study. An incentive of one session of guaranteed child care was ofi’ered. The mothers were asked to complete the questionnaires while at the centers. The packet given to the mothers included a letter fi'om the stafi' at the Family Growth Centers introducing the researcher to the mothers, a letter from the researcher explaining the study, a consent form, a release form that allows the researcher access to the participants' files at the FGCs, the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form, the Maternal Social Support Index, the ._\_. 40 Family Growth Center Questionnaire, and a 3x5 index card (Appendix B, pp 98-102 for letters, consent form, release form, and drawings of the reserved care coupon and the 3x5 card). An identification number was placed on all the questionnaires and the 3x5 card. The index card had a place for the parents to sign their names and to request a summary of the research findings. The completed index card was then placed by the participants in a locked file card box in order to preserve their confidentiality and to allow the researcher to determine which mothers participated in the study when additional information was needed fi-om the mothers’ files at the FGCs. The outside of the questionnaire envelope had a checklist of items that the mother were asked to check to ensure the completeness of the questionnaires (Appendix B, p 103). The envelopes were sealed by the mothers and placed in the box. The researcher attended each respite care session and personally talked to every eligible mother present. It was at this time that the mothers were given a flyer (Appendix B, p 107) explaining the study and were told about the child care coupon. The researcher thoroughly discussed the questionnaire packet with each mother. If the mothers decided to participate, they were asked to sign out a questionnaire packet and were asked if the researcher could contact them by phone or mail, as a reminder, if the packet was not returned in two to three weeks. It was discovemd that the mothers preferred to take home the packet instead of filling it out at the FGCs, hence the sign-out policy. In order to reach all the eligible mothers who had used the centers during the period January 1 to March 14, 1991 but not since then, a greeting card was designed inviting those mothers to try the FGCs again, and to tell them about the study and the reserved care coupon (Appendix B, p 108). 41 These cards were mailed to twenty-one mothers who had used the FGCs since January 1, 1991 and to twenty-eight mothers who had used the FGCs in the Fall of 1990 but had not been back in 1991. Of the forty-nine cards that were mailed, two came back with an incorrect addresses and a no- forwarding address stamp and three were brought in by mothers who asked to participate in the study. At the end of data collection, there was a total of 89 mothers who were . eligible. Of these 89, only six mothers were not contacted either in person or by a greeting card. This represents a contact rate of 93% of all eligible mothers. Sixty-four mothers took a questionnaire packet home. Of these 64 mothers, three mothers declined to complete the questionnaire. One mother felt the questions were too personal, another had just returned fi'om the Gulf War and did not feel she had the time, and the third did not want to complete it for undisclosed reasons. For the other mothers who took a questionnaire home but failed to return them completed, an efi‘ort was made to contact them at least once, but no more than three times. Fifty mothers returned completed questionnaires. This translates to a response rate for all eligible mothers of 56%, with 78% of all mothers who took a questionnaire home returning it completed. Statistical Anubis The data were prepared, cleaned, and entered into the Macintosh statistical program, StatView II (v 1.02, Abacus Concepts, Inc.). In this stage, missing data and unclear responses were examined and decisions on coding them were made. Descriptive statistics of frequency, mean, mode, and variance were used to describe the sample. Zero-order correlations were utilized to answer all three research questions. Trends at 42 .20 to .01 are significant for this exploratory study. The research questions are: 1. What is the relationship between the type and/or number of identified risk factors the mothers had and their level of stress? These risk factors are: (a) low level of income, (1)) low level of education, (c) high number of children, (d) low number of adults residing at home, (e) high number of young children residing at home, (0 minority race status, (g) non-marital status, (h) teen parenthood status, (i) inadequate space in the home, and 6) low number of amenities in the home. 2. What is the relationship between the type and/or number of identified risk factors the mothers had and their level of social support? These risk factors are: (a) low level of income, (b) low level of education, (c) high number of children, ((1) low number of adults residing at home, (e) high number of young children residing at home, (i) minority race status, (g) non-marital status, and (h) teen parenthood status? 3. What is the relationship between the levels of social support and the levels of stress? IimitationsottheSamplingDesignandDataCollectionTechniques The relatively small sample size of fifty mothers does afl‘ect the statistical analysis. With such a small N, power to detect relationships between variables is limited. Even though the sample size is small, it represents 56% of all eligible mothers. This is actually an acceptable response rate for social science research and even exceptional for the high- risk population of mothers who were sampled for this study. The 43 researcher is confident that every efi‘ort was made to ensure that all the mothers who were eligible for the study were included in the sample. The inclusion of those mothers who are foreign students or married to a student may afi‘ect the results of the study. These mothers may score difi'erently on the measures of level of stress and level of social support as a result of their difi‘erent cultures and beliefs as well as their varying levels of acculturation into American culture. Also, many of these mothers live in university housing, where many amenities are provided and the space is only slightly more than adequate. Therefore, it is expected that they will score difi'erently on the PSI-SF and the MSSI and that they will have difi'erent types of risk factors. The researcher separated the foreign mothers from American mothers in the analysis to determine any differences and to answer the research questions. The results will be discussed in Chapter 4. The use of an instrument, the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form, that has no prior use in research and no independent data on reliability and validity may afi‘ect the quality of the data collected. However, this shortened form of the PSI is highly correlated with the PSI and is expected by its author to have similar reliability and validity characteristics. 44 Chapter 4 Results of Statistical Analysis Introduction The results of the statistical analysis of the data will be discussed in this chapter. A description of the sample is given first to provide the necessary information on the demographic characteristics of the sample. The sample was then split into two groups, American and foreign mothers, and Chi-Square and AN OVAs were run to determine the differences between the two groups. Results pertaining to each of the research questions in chapter three will be discussed in the order in which the questions were presented. Each research question will be discussed for both the American and foreign mothers. Cleaning of the Data The sample consisted of fifty mothers who used respite child care during the period of February 11, 1991 to May 10, 1991. Two mothers were dropped fi'om the study due to missing data on the Parenting Stress Index- Short Form (PSI-SF). One mother failed to complete the first page of the PSI-SF and the other mother did not respond to 5 of the 36 items or 14% of ”the questionnaire. There were five other mothers who did not respond to one item and one mother who did not respond to two items on the PSI-SF. A statistical consultant suggested the missing datum for each item be replaced with the mean value of the item for all the participants. This method was chosen 45 because the added values did not affect the placement of the PSI-SF score in the continuum of all PSI-SF scores. All missing values for the risk factors, such as income and number of amenities, rating of the usage of respite child care service, such as length of time and average times a month, and items in the Maternal Social Support Index that were not utilized in the scoring of the instrument were coded as missing data. Reliability of the Imtruments Cronbach’s Alpha using Mean Inter-item Correlation (p) was utiliud to determine the reliability of the PSI-SF with this study’s sample. The equation of Cronbach’s Alpha is: (1: ENE [1+ p(N- 1)] where N is the total number of items in the instrument, 5 is the Mean Inter-item Correlation, and a is the reliability coeficient (Carmines & Zeller, 1979). The range for a is 0 to 1 with a higher (1 indicating higher reliability. The Mean Inter-item Correlation is defined as where n is the total number of items in the instrument, r is the correlation, N is the total number of items in the correlation matrix, and l5 is the mean of all the items in the correlation matrix. 46 The Cronbach’s Alpha for the PSI-SF for this sample was .90. The reliability for each of the three subscales was also computed using the Cronbach’s Alpha. The reliability coefl'rcients for the subscales: Parental Distress, Parent-Child Dysfirnctional Interaction, and Dificult Child were .87, .88, and .78 respectively. The reliabilities of the PSI-SF total score and each of the three subscales for this sample are acceptable. The reliability for the MSSI was not computed due to the difi‘erent levels of measurement used in the instrument. The M881 utilizes nominal, ordinal, and ratio measures. Because the Cronbach’s Alpha determines how reliable maple are in answering the questions, the levels of measurement need to be similar in order for the reliability coeficient to be an accurate measure of the index’s reliability. In the literature, only test- retest reliabilities are listed for this instrument (Pascoe et al., 1988; Pascoe & French, 1990). However, a Cronbach’s Alpha was computed for the two clusters: Child Care Tasks and Non-Child Care Tasks. The reliability coemcients for the clusters were .60 for the Child Care Tasks and .45 for the the Non-Child Care Tasks. Though these are low, the alpha for the Child Care Cluster is in the range reported in the literature and the alpha for the N on-Child Care Cluster is slightly less than the range reported (Pascoe et al., 1988). Possible reasons for these low correlations may be due to the small sample size and very small number of items in the correlation matrix. Only three items comprise the Child Care Tasks cluster and seven items comprise the second cluster. 47 Description of the Sample The sample consisted of 48 mothers who used respite child care during the spring of 1991. The mean age for the mothers was 30.5 years (SD = 5.29) with a range of 20 to 42 years. The sample was highly educated with a mean of 14.4 years/(SD = 2.45) and a mode of 16 to 17+ years. As illustrated in Figure 4-1, the sample represents an array of ethnic groups and races, with a majority of the women being Caucasian. The sample is mostly poor, with 59.4% of the women classified as being below the poverty . threshold (Table 4-1). A large portion (75%) of the women in the study are married and the same percentage of women cite two adults in the household. 'leenty-three percent of the mothers listed themselves as the only adult in the home. a Caucasian 58.3% In Asian 16.7% Black 10.4% I Hispanic 6.25% I Middle Eastern 8.3% Figure 4-1. Description of the study sample by ethnicity and race. 48 The average number of children for this sample was 2.2 (SD = 1.2), with 75% of the mothers having either one or two children. All/of the mothers had children who were 5 years or younger, with 50% of the mothers having one child and 39.6% of the mothers having two children in this age category. Men mothers had children between the ages of six and eleven years and only thme mothers had children who were thiMen years or older. Table 4-1. Frequency distribution of income for the study sample. Gatorade: Humbeufmpls fiercest Non-poverty 15 4054 Poverty threshold 4 10.81 20% below poverty 2 5.41 40% below poverty 10 27.03 60% below poverty 5 13.51 80% below poverty 1 2.70 The study asked the mothers about the amount of space and the - number of amenities in their home. Forty-eight percent of the mothers classified the amount of space in their home as below the “just enough” category on a seven point Likert scale. All of the mothers have running water, electricity, heat, a refiigerator, and a stove/oven. Three mother did not have telephones and seven women did not have cars. Slightly more than half of the mothers did not have a washer or dryer. 49 In determining the mothers’ usage of the respite child care service, it was found that 50% of the mothers have been using the center for less than one year. The average length of time the mothers have been using the service is between seven and nine months. Forty-one percent of the mothers use the centers an average of four times a month and 30.1% use the centers only 1 to 3 times a month. The mothers’ levels of social support and stress were measured. The sample’s mean score on the MSSI was 21.6 (SD = 6.5) with a range of 7 to 34. The maximum score on this instrument is 39. It was found that the mothers were experiencing high levels of stress. The mean for the Total Stress score was 81.4 (SD = 16.8) with a range of 42 to 118. A score of 90 or above represents a mother who is experiencing clinically high level of stress. Thirteen mothers in this sample had scores of 90 or above. A mother in this sample is most likely to be Caucasian, married, and living below the poverty threshold. She has a high school diploma and has possibly completed a year or two of college. She has two children with at least one who is 5 years or younger. In her home, she has all the amenities except for a washer and dryer and she has slightly less than just enough space for her family. She is experiencing a high level stress and a moderate level of social support. Diflermrces between the American and Foreign Mothers The inclusion of the foreign mothers who use the third center introduwd the possibility that there were significant difl‘erences between these mothers and the American mothers in the sample. Therefore, the sub-groups were compared to determine any difi'erences. The sample of 48 mothers was split into two groups: American and foreign. Chi-square and 50 AN OVAs were run to determine any significant difi'erences between the two groups. The American sub-sample consisted of 32 mothers and the foreign sub-sample had 16 mothers. Between the two groups, there were eleven significant difi‘erences below the .05 level and three between the .10 and .05 probability level. They are liswd in Table 4-2. The foreign mothers tended to have lower social support scores. A difi‘erence of 3.3 points exists between the means of the two groups. The foreign mothers average score was 19.5. However, these mothers were also likely to have higher scores on the two MSSI clusters: Child Care Tasks and N on-Child Care Tasks. The average score was 1.9 for the Child Care Tasks cluster and 4.0 for the Non-Child Care Tasks. The mean difi'erences were .781 and .969, respectively. All sixteen foreign mothers were married compared to only 62.5% of the American mothers. The foreign mothers all had one other adult besides themselves in the household. Only 21 or 62.5% of' the Americans listed two adults in the home. None of the foreign mothers had children before the age of twenty, compared to seven American mothers who were teen parents. There are difi'erences in the number and ages of the children between less two groups of mothers. Based on the mean difi'erences between the /:o sub-groups, the American mothers had on the average one more child ( than the foreign mothers and .344 more children in the age group of five l l \,\ years or younger. Besides difi'erences in the number of people in the home, there are difi‘erences in the number of amenities and amount of space. It was found that the foreign mothers have fewer amenities than the American mothers. Also, the foreign mothers tended to rate their amount of space as not being 51 .8. v a: no. v a . 3. v a I. .24. SE a 8.3 re 8 o 5% SE 2..” 8d :3. 13d 3 a: a: 83:5 as a tame EN 84 new cote—Eu no a tame m3 m3 2: 8:8ch | .8 a :43. alone Br 88 3.33 e e +53 8.3 3.2 ES 938 an: In}. new 8... 8.» 2:3 28 6:50-:0: am: :Se «3 x: m: sacs «.30 2mg“. .mmz .....§.om 5: 3.x lemme secs: In; 3.3 8.3 q: 558:3 tame Re are 8e 82: e unnoE< 38,—. m seesaw ~38. system "83.88471. :33. ammouom canton}. $33.29. .30 3:8qu aces: 6.85:2 sunfish e5 deer—23w 23 8253 80:98me .er 03am. 52 enough for their family. There was a mean difi’erence of 1.25 between the sub-groups in the variable of amount of space. The foreign mothers on the whole, were more educated. They received 1.3 more years of schooling then the American mothers. Whereas the American mothers had an average level of education of 14 years, the foreign mothers went to school an average of 15.3 years. Fifty percent of the foreign mothers are classified as Asian, 25% as Middle Eastern, 12.5% as black and 12.5% as Caucasian. This compares to the American sample which is 81.2% Caucasian, 9.3% Black, and 9.3% Hispanic. There was a difference in the length of time that the two groups of mothers have used the respite child care service. The American mothers were likely to have used the center longeruan average of eight months longer. The average for the American mothers was 16 months. There were no difi‘erences in the number of times a month the mothers used the centers. Both groups’ average was 4.4 times a month. The foreign mothers in this sample had higher levels of education when compared to the American mothers. All of the foreign mothers were married and tended to have fewer children. These mothers tended to have less space and fewer amenities in their homes. Though they receive more help with every day tasks including child care, they have lower social support scores. As a result of the findings of significant difi‘erences between the American and foreign mothers, the sample was split into two sub-groups and separate analyses were done for each group. The findings of the study are discussed by the research questions posed m Chapter 3 and by the two sub-groups of American and foreign mothers. Results of the Stark W: What is the relationship between the type and/or number of identified risk factors the mothers had and their level of stress? Theisfloflrisk .. factors are: (a) low level of mcome, (b) low level of education, (c) high number of children, (d) low number of adults residing at home, (e) high number of young children residing at home, (0 minority race status, (g) non-marital status, (h) teen parenthood status, (i) inadequate space in the home, and 6) low number of amenities in the home. Zero-order correlations were used to answer this question. The identified risk factors were correlated with the mothers’ scores on the PSI- SF (Table 4.3). The results of the correlational analyses are discussed for the American sub-group first followed by the foreign sub-group. TW‘ American Sub-group The results mostly show only slight relationships between the risk factors and the mothers’ level of stress for the American sub-group. A positive relationship was found for the risksfgoooréofi ngzrber of children and the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF (r- — 27; p <. 20). The m thers with more children had higher levels of stress. Another positive relationship was found for the numberyd of adults m the home and the mothers' PSI-SF scores (r = .1;? p < ..20) Las wyfmoihers with fewer amenities were experiencing higher levels of stress (r = .21; p < .20). The most significant finding for this sub-group was that the number of children the mother had that were five years or younger was related to the mothers' scores on the PSI-SF (r = .45; p < .05). The more young children the mother had, the higher her score on the PSI-SF. 54 Table 4-3. Correlations between the Risk Factors and the Mothers’ PSI- SF Scores by Sub-groups and Total Sample. Income - .016 -.26+ -.087 Education -.081 -.23+ -.099 # of children .27+ -.34+ .13-t- # of adults ’ .17+ —- .15+ # of children s 5 years .45" -.36+ .21+ Minority Status .14 .27+ .18+ Single-parent - Status -.O76 -- -.086 Teen parent Status .066 - .029 Amount of space .038 .053 .011 # of amenities .21+ .17 .15+ """ + '52?2'o"""""""""'-"'"""""""""'" "' p < .05 There were no significant findings for the following risk factors: income, mmofity status, single-parent status, teen-parent status, and amount of space. Foreign Sub-group h; Correlations were run for the risk factors and the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF for the foreign sub-group. There were three risk factors: single- parent, number of adults, and teen parent, which did not apply for this sub- group. The mothers were all married; therefore there are two adults in every home and none of the foreign mothers were under the age of twenty at the birth of their first child. Two of the risk factors had unexpected correlations with the stress scores at the .20 level. Both number of children and nomber of children 6‘. 7”,. L455, / WI} {QM Ni 0 (:1 C27 55 who are S 5 years of age were negatively related to the foreign mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF (rs = -.34 and -.36, respectively). For these mothers, . the more children they had, the lower their stress scores. All other risk factors were related to the mothers’ stress scores and were in the emcted direction. These risk factors are: income (r = -.26; p < .20), education (r = .24; p < .20), and minority status (r = .27; p < .20). Foreign mothers who were either poorer or less educated had higher stress scores. The fourteen foreign mothers who classified themselves as minorities had higher PSI-SF scores. The amount of, spgogrggd number of amenth ggmfiwfly related to the foreign mothers’ levels of stress. Summary The data indicate that all but one of the significant correlations are at the .20 level for both sub-groups. For the American sub-group, the number of children, adults, and amenities were all positively related to the mothers’ levels of stress. Those American mothers who had more children, more adults and more amenities in the home had higher stress scores. It is surprising to find that the American mothers who were single—parents were experiencing lower levels of stress. The most significant finding for the American mothers was that the number of children they had that were five years of age or younger was positively related to their levels of stress (r s .445; p < .05). For the foreign mothers sub-group, all significant correlations were at the .20 level. These risk factors that were negatively related to the foreign mothers’ PSI-SF scores were income, education, number of children, and number of children five years or younger. The only risk factor that was 56 positively related to the mothers’ levels of stress was minority status. Number of adults, single-parent status, and teen parent status did not apply to this sub-group, since all the mothers were married and none were teen parents. W: What is the relationship between the type and/or number of . identified risk factors the mothers had and their level of social support? These risk factors are: (a) low level of income, (b) low level of education, (c) high number of children, (d) low number of adults residing at home, (e) high number of young children residing at home, (0 minority race status, (g) non-marital status, and (h) teen parenthood status? Correlations] analyses of the risk factors and the mothers’ scores on the MSSI were used to answer the second research question. The results of the analyses are discussed by sub-groups beginning with the American mothers, followed by the foreign mothers. The results are liswd in Table 4-4. American Sub-group A significant finding of this study is that the American mothers with lower levels of social support were found to have lower levels of income (r s -.56, p < .01). This is a negative relationship because the higher the number assigned to the category of income, the farther they are from the poverty threshold. Another negative relationship. was found for the risk factor of single-parent status and the mothers’ scores on the MSSI. The single-parent mothers in this subgroup had lower levels of support compared to the married mothers (r = -.36; p < .05). 57 Table 4-4. Correlations between the Risk Factors and the Mothers’ MSSI Scores by Sub-groups and Total Sample. Income - .56" -.13 -.47** Education .27+ .36+ .21 if of children .031 -.32+ .079 # of adults .044 -- -.01 # of children s 5 years -.081 .009 .001 Minority status -.19+ .17 -.23 Single-parent Status -.36* -- -.18 Teen parent Status -.20+ -- -.088 +p<20 * p < .05 *" p < .01 The other significant findings for the American sub—group were all at the .20 level. There was a positive relationship between the risk factor of education and the mothers’ scores on the MSSI (r a: .27). The mothers with the fewest years of schooling had lower levels of social support. A mother who listed herself as a minority or was a teen parent tended to have lower levels of social support (rs = -.19 and -.20, respectively). the number of adults or children in the home regardless of their ages were not significantly related to the American mothers’ levels of social support. Foreign Sub-group The foreign mothers were similar to the American mothers on the risk factor of education (r = .36; p < .20). The scatter graph for level of education and mothers’ scores on the MSSI shows a curvilinear 58 relationship, as illustrated in Figure 4-2. Those mothers with low or high levels of education had higher levels of social support compared to those mothers with moderate levels of education. For this sample, moderate levels of education is approximately 13 to 16 years of schooling. 0000 A L MSSSITOTALSCORE SEKSEBE§888 O 0 1'2 125 T4 1'5 1'6 1'7 133 EDUCATION H H Figure 4-2. Scatterplot of the relationship between the MSSI scores and level of education for the foreign subogroup. The only other significant finding for this sub-group was the negative relationship between the foreign mothers’ scores on the MSSI and the risk factor, number of children. The foreign mothers who had more children had lower social support scores (r = -.36; p < .20). Income, number of children five years or younger, and minority status were not significantly related to the foreign mothers’ levels of social support. The risk factors; number of adults, single-parent status, and teen parent status did not apply 59 to this sub-group, since all the mothers were married and none were teen parents. Summary The results of this study indicate that the majority of the risk factors that were relawd to the mothers’ score on the MSSI for both the American and foreign sub-groups were at the .20 level. Education, minority status, and teen parent status were significantly related to the American mothers’ levels of social support (rs = .27, -.19, and -.36, respectively). For the foreign mothers, the risk factors of education and number of children were significantly related to their levels of social support (rs = .36 and -.32, respectively). There were two risk factors that were highly related to the American mothers’ scores on the MSSI. They are single-parent status (r = -.36; p < .05) and income (r = -.56; p < .01). The We risk factors: number of adults, single-parent, and teen parent, did not apply for the foreign sub- group, because there were no differences in these risk factors for them. W: What is the relationship between the levels of social support and the levels of stress? ’ Zero-order correlations were used to answer question three. The mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF and its three sub-scales were correlated with the mother’s scores on the MSSI, their scores on the MSSI clusters, and two variables of mothers’ usage of the respite child care service; number of months and times a month (Table 4-5). The results of the correlations! analyses are discussed for the American sub-group first, followed by the foreign sub—group. 8. v Am... 3. v Ar... 8. v .r *3. +3. +3.- Km. wmor EN. +3.- ++mmz 5.82 a 385. 2. ma. 3.. vo. S.- Ec m7 $6. 3362 we a “mo.- mfi. 8o. m8. +3".- Er Na: «8.- 00.202 Hmmz +5.- H. 9.- v8.- 2: S.- +wmc m8.- 00 mag Eb. +5. 30.- mmov ..omr ..er +3.- m8.- ~wm2 :mmfioh 58th< ammouoh swam-8:2 camps-m user—25‘ ago-m 5.33:5. fiommam l r Emeom Savanna 0Q oiomnsm HOD-m eigensm Gm ram-Em anew-paw 5e: .8.“ 38.5 mo .33 was $5302 tandem 38m 23 5953 nEuqeflaEm 25. .m-v Snag. American Sub-group In this sub-group, the PSI-SF was positively related to the number of times a month the mother used the respite child care service (r=.33; p<.10). Those mothers who had higher stress scores used the service more times on the average during the month. There was a significant relationship between the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF subscale, Parental Distress and their scores on the MSSI (r a -.42, p < .05). The lower the mother's level of social support, the higher her level of distress as a parent. The Parental Distress subscale was positively related to the number of times a month she used the service (r = .25; p <.20). The more stress a mother was experiencing in her parental role, the more often on average that she used the service. The mothers scores on the Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction (P-CDI) subscale were positively related to the number of times \a month the mother used the respite child care service (r = .37; p < .05). Those mothers who had a child that did not measure up to their idealized child or did not positively reinforce them in their parenting role used the center more often in a months’ time. The mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF subscale, Dificult Child, was positively related to the mothers’ scores on the MSSI and the number of times a month the mothers used the service at the .20 level (rs :- .27 and .17, respectively). A mother who was raising a child who was dimcult had a higher level of social support and she used the service more often in a months’ time than a mother who was not raising a dificult child. Foreign Sub-group The mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF were negatively related to three of the measures of social support. Those mothers who had used the respite child care services more times during a month’s time had lower levels of stress (r = -.34; p < .20). The mothers who had higher levels of social support as measured by the MSSI, had lower levels of stress (r s -.29; p < .20 ). Lastly, those mothers who received more help with child care tasks as measured by the MSSI Child Care Cluster had lower scores on the PSI-SF (r = -.28; p < .20). The mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF subscale, Parental Distress (PD) was negatively related to their levels of social support as measured by the MSSI (r a -.50, p < .05). Those mothers who scored higher on the PD subscale received less help with household tasks as measured by the MSSI Non-Child Care Cluster (r a -.34; p < .20). Those mothers who had lower levels of social support and who received less help with household tasks had higher levels of distress as a parent. A negative relationship was also found for the mothers’ scores on the subscale, Parent-Child Dysfimctional Interaction (P-CDI) and the number of times a month the mothers’ used the respite child care service (r = -.32: p < .20). Those mothers who had a child that did not measure up to their idealized child or did not positively reinforce them in their parenting role used the service less often than those mothers who were receiving positive reinforcement in their parent role. As with the American sub-group, there was a positive relationship between the mothers’ scores on the subscale, Dificult Child (DC) and the number of times a month the mothers used the service (r a .49; p < .05). Mothers who rated their children as more difficult used the service more w often. The mothers’ scores on the DC were negatively related to. the mothers’ scores on the MSSI Child Care Tasks Cluster (r = -.27; p < .20). Those mothers who rated their children as more dificult received less help with child care tasks than those mothers who rated their children as being easy. Summary For the American sub-group, those mothers who used the respite child care service more frequently were experiencing higher levels of stress (r s .33; p < .20). The opposite relationship was found for the foreign sub- group. Those foreign mothers who used the service more frequently were experiencing lower levels of stress (r a -.34; p < .10). For only the foreign mothers, there were significant relationships between the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF and her scores on the MSSI and the MSSI Child Care Clusters (rs - -.29 and -.28, respectively both at the .20 level). Another negative relationship was found between both sub-groups’ scores on the PSI-SF subscale, Parental Distress and their scores on the MSSI. This finding was true for both sub-groups at the .05 level. American mothers who were experiencing distress in their parental role were using the center more mquently (r . .25; p < .20). The foreign mothers’ scores on the PD were also negatively related to the mothers’ scores on the MSSI Non- Child Care Cluster. For both sub-groups, there was a significant relationship between the mothers’ scores on the P-CDI Subscale and the number of times a month they used the service. For the American mothers, the relationship was positive and it was a negative relationship for the foreign mothers (r a .37; p < .05 and r s -.32; p < .20, respectively). 64 The American mothers’ scores on the Dificult Child Subscale were positively correlated with the mothers scores on the MSSI and the number of times a month they used the service (rs = .27 and .17, respectively). Both relationships were significant at the .20 level. The foreign mothers’ scores on the DC were positively correlated with the number of times a month they used the service (r = .49; p < .05). The foreign mothers’ scores on the DC were negatively related to their scores on the MSSI Child Care Cluster (r = -.27; p < .20). Chapter Summary , Statistically significant difi'erences were found between the Amefimmtheflfogign‘ mothers diagram-i: variables: MSSI total score, MSSI Child Care Tasks cluster, MSSI Non-Child Care Task cluster, number of adults, number of amenities, number of children, number of children s five years of age, education, marital status, single-parent, number of months using the respite child care, minority status, amount of space, and men parent status. Thflothem were separated into two groups and analyses were run to answer the research questions. Statistically significant findings for this study between the type or number of risk factors and the mothers’ score on the PSI-SF and MSSI were found for both the American and foreign sub-groups. In answering research question one, a positive relationship was found for the American mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF and the following risk factors; number of children, number of children who were five years of age or younger, number of adults, and number of amenities. For the foreign mothers, their scores on the PSI-SF were negatively related to the following risk factors; income, education, number of children, and number of children 65 who are five years of age or younger. The risk factor of ethnidty/race was positively relawd to the foreign mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF. In answering research question two, it was found that the American and foreign mothers’ scores on the MSSI was positively related to the risk factor of education. Negative relationships were found between the American mothers’ scores on the MSSI and the following risk factors; single-parent status, teen parent status, ethnidty/race, and income. For the foreign mothers, there were negative relationships between the mothers’ scores on the MSSI and two risk factors; number of children, and number of children five years of age or younger. Research question three examined the relationship between the mothers’ levels of stress and their levels of soda] support . For the American mothers, the number of times a month the mother used the service was positively related to the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF and all three of its subscales. The American mothers’ scores on the MSSI were negatively related to the mothers’ scores on the Parental Distress subscale and positively related to the mothers’ scores on the Dificult Child subscale. For the foreign mothers, times a month they used the service. was negatively related to the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF and the Parent- Child Dysfunctional Interaction subscale. The variable, times a month, i was positively related to the foreign mothers’ scores on the Dificult Child subscale. The foreign mothers’ scores on the MSSI were negatively related to the mothers scores on the PSI-SF and the Parental Distress subscale. There were negative relationships between the foreign mothers’ scores on the MSSI Child Care Cluster and their scores on the PSI-SF and the Dificult Child subscale. Lastly, a negative relationship between the $ foreign mothers’ scores on the MSSI N on-Child Care Cluster and the Parental Distress subscale was found. 67 Chapter 5 Discussion and Implications Introduction The main objectives of this study were: 1) to determine if the identified risk factors were related to the sample's maternal level of stress, 2) to determine if the identified risk factors were related to the sample's maternal level of soda] support, and 3) to determine if the maternal level of soda] support was related to the sample's level of maternal stress. In this chapter, the results of this study are discussed in terms of these research objectives. This is followed by suggestions for future research. Dlflsrences within the Sample The sample was composed of two sub-groups; American mothers and foreign mothers who were living in married, student housing at a midwestern university. The American sub-group included 32 mothers and the foreign sub-group consisted of 16 mothers. Chi-square and AN OVAs revealed significant difi‘erences between the two groups. The foreign mothers had lower levels of soda] support as measured by the Maternal Sodal Support Index (MSSI). Even though the foreign mothers scored lower on the MSSI, they scored higher on its two clusters, Child Care Tasks and Non-Child Care tasks. The foreign mothers received more help with everyday tasks, which may be a result of the fact that all of the foreign mothers were married compared to only 20 of the American mothers. Their lower scores on the MSSI are probably a result of their isolation from relatives and lack of involvement in groups or organizations. E The foreign mothers had higher levels of stress as measured by the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form (PSI-SF). These mothers also scored - higher on two of the three subscales: Parental Distress and Parent-Child Interaction. “Parental Distress subscale (PD) measures the stress the mother feels in her role as a parent as a function of personal factors that are positively related to parenting" (Abidin, 1990, p. 19). One of these personal factors is lack of soda] support. A possible explanation for their higher scores on the PD is that the foreign mothers have younger children and as such, are still making the transition into the parenting role. They are making this transition without support from their extended family and with little or no outside support in the form of groups or organizations. The foreign mothers had fewer risk factors than the American mothers. The few risk factors they had were: fewer amenities in the home, lack of adequate space, more likely to be classified as a minority, and lower levels of income. All of these foreign mothers live in married student housing. Even though the small apartments have many amenities, they do not have washers and dryers. The American mothers had more room in their homes and possessed washers and dryers. All mothers in the sample had running water, electridty, and kitchen appliances, and only three American mothers had no telephones. The foreign mothers were most likely to be Asian, Black, or Middle Eastern. The majority of the foreign mothers had income levels between 40 to 80% below the poverty threshold, whereas the majority of the American mothers had incomes that were either at poverty threshold or above it. Despite having a higher number of risk factors (six out of ten)- the American mothers had lower levels of stress and higher levels of support. The American mothers were more likely to be involved in outside 69 organizations and groups, frequently see relatives, and have been using the respite child care services for a longer period of time. Though the majority of the American mothers were married, twelve mothers were single- parents and there were seven American teen mothers in the study. The American mothers had lower levels of education, a higher number of children, and more children who were S 5 years of age. The research objectives are discuswd together for each subcsamme of this study. When there are difi‘erences in the findings for the sub-groups, 'they will be discussed separately. Disarssion of the Research Olfiectives Wm: To determine if the identified risk factors were related to the sample's maternal level of stress. For the American sub-sample, the number of children s 5 years of age was related to the amount of stress the mothers were experiendng. The mothers with high levels of stress had more young children. This finding supports the observations of Miller and Myers-Walls (1983) who feel that mothers with young children experience more stress. It also supports the definition of Pittman, Wright, and Llyod (1989) for this variable as a stressor in their research. They defined the presence of preschoolers as a relevant background factor that contributes to the parents’ levels of stress. However, this result is not supportive of the results found by Glenn and Weaver (1979). In their study, they found that the number of children between the ages of 0-5 years was negatively related to the father’s well- being but not to the mothers. One possible reason for the discrepancy is that these researchers examined the relationship between the ages of children and parents’ measure of well-being, whereas, this study examined the 7O relationship between the ages of children and the mothers’ level of stress. Well-being and stress are measuring two difi‘erent ends of a continuum and a decrease in a person’s score of well-being is not indicative of an increase in their level of stress. Contrary to the findings for the American sub—group, the foreign mothers who had more children between the ages of infancy through five years had lower levels of stress. This finding is supportive of the results found by Glenn and Weaver (1979). However, it is not supportive of the literature in which this risk factor is defined as a stressor (Pittman, Wright, & Lloyd, 1989). The risk factors of low level of education and income were both negatively related to the foreign mothers’ levels of stress. The finding that mothers with lower levels of education had higher levels of stress is supportive of the definition of this risk factor as a stressor in the literature (Conger et al., 1984; McLanahan, 1983; Zuravin, 1988). It is also supportive of a study involving black, single-parent mothers. The researchers found that low levels of education were related to higher levels of stress (Lindblad- Goldberg, Dukes, & Lasely, 1988). The finding that mothers with low levels of income had higher levels of stress is also supportive of the literature (Andrews & Withey, 1979; McLanahan, 1983; Makosky, 1982; Pearlin & Johnson, 1977). McLanahan (1983) found a negative relationship between level of income and number of stressful life events experienced by the family. In another study, 43 low- income mothers were found to experience more stressful life events (Makosky, 1982). In a study of people from the Chicago area, economic strain was positively related to the level of depression (Pearlin & Johnson, 1977). Lastly, Andrews and Withey (1979) found that people with lower 71 levels of income expressed more negative evaluations of the following concerns: economic, family and marriage, health, and fun and enjoyment. In this study, other identified risk factors such as number of children, number of adults, and number of amenities were positively related to the American mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF. Mothers with more children had higher levels of stress. This finding is supported in the literature (McLanahan, 1985; Nye, Carlson, & Garrett, 1970; Pearlin and Johnson, 1977). McLanahan (1985) found that parents who had more children experienwd a decrease in their level of well-being over a three year period. Nye, Carlson, and Garrett (1970) found an increase in mothers’ levels of stress after the birth of the second child. Lastly, Pearlin and Johnson (197 7) found a positive relationship between the number of children and the parents’ levels of stress. A finding of this study that is contradictive of findings reported in the literature, is that the number of adults in the home was positively related to the American mothers’ levels of stress. In the literature, those mothers that are single-parents (the only adult in the home) experience higher levels of stress (McLanahan, 1985; Weinraub & Wolf, 1983). McLanahan (1985) found that the well-being of single-mothers declined by 11% over a one year period. In a study conducted by Weinraub and Wolf (1983), it was found that the single-parents experienced more life changes than the parents who were married. One of the reasons for the positive relationship between single- parenthood and level of stress is that single-parents are more likely to also be experiendng the following risk factors; income, education, and minority status. In this study, all but one of the mothers who were single-parents listed themselves as the only adult in the home. Therefore, when 72 discussing the risk factor, number of adults, all but one of the mothers who listed two adults or more in the home were married. These single-parent mothers had lower levels of education compared to the married mothers by an average of two years. Their average income level was 50% below poverty level compared to 10% below for the married mothers. These single mothers were not all minorities. In fact, all but one of the mothers were Caucasian. McLanahan (1985) found that single-parenthood was more stressful for whit mothers than for black mothers. Even though the single- parent mothers had more risk factors they were experiendng lower levels of stress. For the foreign mothers, a positive relationship between ethnidty/race and level of stress was found. This relationship is supported . in the literature (Andrews & Withey, 1976; Conger et al., 1984; McAdoo, 1983; Pearlin & Johnson, 1977). As with single—parenthood, race is assodated with an increased likelihood of having more risk factors. These risk factors are; education, income, single-parent status, and number of children. W: To determine if the identified risk factors were related to the sample's maternal level of soda] support. * For the American mothers, level of income, ethnidty/race, single- parent status, and teen parent status were all negatively related to the mothers’ level of soda] support. Those mothers who were either poor and/or a single-parent had lower levels of support. This is suppomd in the literature. Both McLanahan (1983) and Weinraub and Wolf (1983) found that single-parent mothers had less contact with their extended families, reported receiving less support from them, and were less likely to know 73 their neighbors. D’Ercole (1988) found that single mothers who had higher levels of support experienced less finanda] strain due to the stressor of low - income. American mothers who were classified as minority were more likely to be experiendng lower levels of sodal support. American mothers who were teen parents had lower levels of soda] support. These mothers who were teen parents are probably younger than their children’s fiiends parents and are out of step with women their own age. As a result, they may find themselves with a support network composed of members that are not very similar to themselves. Studies have found that sodal support fi-om network members is more efi‘ective if the members are similar to the receiver of support (D'Ercole, 1988; Gladow & Ray, 1986; Zur-Szpiro & Lonflellow, 1982). For both sub-groups there was a positive relationship between the mothers’ levels of soda] support and their level of education. For the American mothers, the more schooling she completed, the more support she was receiving. These more educated mothers may be more aware of the importance of soda] support and of the services available for them and may therefore actively seek them. The foreign mothers, who had only moderate levels of education compared to the other foreign mothers, were receiving a lower amount of soda] support. The foreign mothers who had only moderate levels of education may be dissimilar to those mothers who have either low or high levels of education. Those with the low levels are probably in Michigan while their husbands go to school and there may be a large number of mothers in this similar position. The same trend may exist for the mothers with high levels of education. That is, these mothers represent a large group of women who are enrolled in school and pursuing a Ph.d. This would be 74 supportive of the work done by D'Ercole (1988), Gladow and Ray (1986). and Zur-Szpiro and Longfellow (1982). For only the foreign mothers, a negative relationship was found between the number of children and level of soda] support. The explanation that the more children a parent has, the less time he/she has to maintain and develop his/her sodal networks may be plausible (Miller & Myer-Walls, 1983). The foreign mothers had younger children compared to the American mothers. Therefore, they may be experiendng higher 'demands on their time and energy because of this lower age and thus have less time with member of their sodal network. W: To determine if the sample’s level of soda] support was related to the sample's maternal level of stress. For the foreign sub-group, the mothers’ scores on the MSSI total and the MSSI Child Care Tasks Cluster were negatively related to the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF. Also for this sub-group the mothers’ scores on the Parental Distress subscale were negatively related to her scores on the Non- child Care Tasks Cluster. Mothers with higher levels of soda] support had lower levels of stress. The foreign mothers scores on the Dificult child subscale were negatively related to her scores on the MSSI Child Care Tasks. Those mothers who were experiendng stress due to raising a difficult child received less help with child care tasks. These findings are supported in the literature by the Bufi'ering Hypothesis (Wilcox, 1981) and research on the mediating efi'ects of soda] support (D’Ercole, 1988; Gladow & Ray, 1986; Zur-Szpiro & Longfellow, 1982). According to the Bufi'ering Hypothesis, sodal support protects people against the efi‘ects of stressful life events on the person’s well-being (Wilcox, 75 1981). Zur-Szpiro and Longfellow ( 1982) found that the help with child care tasks, household tasks, and emotional support were negatively related to the mothers’ levels of stress. Gladow and Ray (1986) and D’Ercole (1988) found that the support a single-parent mother receives from her family and friends was positively related to her level of well-being. The negative relationship between the American and foreign mothers’ scores on the subscale, Parental Distress and their scores on the MSSI is also supported by this literature. It was surprising to find contradicting findings for the two sub- groups on the relationship between the mothers’ usage of respite child care services and their levels of stress as measured by the PSI-SF and two of its three subscales, Parental Distress, and Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction. The American mothers who had used the service longer or more frequently had higher levels of stress. This is not supported in the research literature focusing on respite child care. What is supported is the negative relationship between usage of the service and level of stress for the foreign sub-group as measured by the PSI-SF and the Parent-child Dysfunctional Interaction subscale. In three empirical research studies, there was a decrease in the mothers’ level of stress among users of respite child care (Rimmerman, 1989; Subramanian, 1985; Wikler, Hanusa, & Stoychefi', 1986). These three studies used a pre- and post-test methodology that was not utilized in this study. Where causation can be inferred for those three studies, it can not be inferred from this study. There is a positive relationship between the frequency of American mothers’ usage of the service and their levels of stress as measured by the PSI-SF and its three subscales and the foreign mothers frequency of usage of the service and the 76 Dificult child subscale. It could be that those American mothers with the higher stress scores were aware of being under a high level of stress and intentionally sought the respite child care services. As for the foreign mothers, those mothers who are raising a dimcult child may be more willing to use the service than those mothers who rate their children easier to rear. This may also be an explanation for the positive relationship between the American mothers’ scores on the MSSI and the Dificult Child subscale. American mothers who were raising diffith children were receiving more support from their network members. Not only are American mothers possibly more willing to use support services but also to receive support from friends and family members. Summary The general conclusion of this study was that there were relationships among the identified risk factors, mothers’ levels of stress, and their levels of soda] support. Due to statistically significant difi'erences between the American and the foreign mothers on the soda] support measure, usage of respite child care service, and several risk- factors, separate analyses were run to meet the research objectives. Those American mothers who had higher levels of stress had a higher number of; children, children who were five years of age or younger, adults in the home, and amenities in the home. Little or no relationship was found for I the American mothers’ levels of stress and the following risk factors; level of income, level of education, minority status, single-parent status, teen parent status, and amount of space. 77 The foreign mothers who had higher levels of stress had lower levels of income and education and fewer number of children and number of children who are five years of age or younger. Those foreign mothers who were Asian or black had higher levels of stress. There was little or no relationship found for the foreign mothers’ levels of stress and the risk factors of number of amenities and amount of space. There was no relationship between their levels of stress or soda] support and the risk factors of single-parent status, teen parent status, and number of adults. American mothers who had higher levels of education had higher levels of soda] support . For the foreign mothers, lower and higher levels of education were related to higher levels of sodal support. American mothers who had lower levels of sodal support had the following risk factors; single-parent status, teen parent status, ethnidty/race, and income. Little or no relationship was found between the American mothers’ levels of soda] support and the following risk factors; number of children, number of children S five years of age, and number of adults. The foreign mothers who had lower levels of soda] support had a higher number of children and number of children five years of age or younger. The risk factor, level of income, was only slightly related to the foreign mothers’ levels of soda] support. Relationships were found between the measures of soda] support and stress for both sub-groups. For the American mothers, the more stress a mother was experiendng as measured by the PSI-SF and all three of its subscales, the more frequently the mother used the service during a months’ time. The American mothers who received more support reported that they were experiendng less stress in their parent role and rated their child as being difficult. There was little or no relationship 78 found between the mothers’ scores on the PSI-SF and its three subscales for either the two MSSI clusters and the number of months the mothers used the respite child care service. The more fi'equently the foreign mothers used the service, the less stress they were experiendng as measured by the PSI-SF and the Parent- Child Dysfunctional Interaction subscale. These same mothers rated their child as being dificult to raise. The foreign mothers who were experiendng higher levels of stress and parental distress had less soda] support available to them. Those mothers who received less help with child care tasks were experiendng more stress and rated their child as being dificult. Lastly, those mothers who received less help with non-child care tasks reported experiendng distress in their parental role. Little or no relationship was found between the PSI-SF and its three subscales and the number of months the foreign mothers used the respite child care service. Future Research Because of the significant difi'erences found between the American and foreign mothers, a research study with more foreign mothers would provide additional information that would provide greater detail on those differences. A limitation of this study was the small number of foreign mothers, which resulted in combining both the American and foreign mothers into one group. Consequently, Asian, Black, and Middle Eastern mothers were all placed into one sub-group. A study with a larger number of Asian and Middle Eastern mothers may provide data on the differences within the foreign sub-group population. The finding that the American mothers who use the respite child care service more are experiendng higher levels of stress is contrary to the 79 research in this area. A pre-and post-test methodological study with a sample from the same centers would provide the additional information needed to determine why these mothers are experiendng higher levels of stress. In order to increase the sample size, the pre-test and the data collection time period should be at least one year. This should increase the sample size and possibly include those mothers who are only seasonal users of the service. Lastly, the respite child care service examined in this study was ' deve10ped to reduce the possibility and the occurrence of child abuse. Therefore, a longitudinal study that would begin when the mothers start to use the service and follow them for several years after their last visit would provide valuable information on whether or not the service meets its purpose. This study should include interviews with the parents, other family members and child care providers as well as standardized tests and possibly protective service reports and records. REFERENCES REFERENCES Abidin, R. (1990) ° Charlottesville, Virginia: Pediatric Psychology Press. Abidin- R. (1986). W Charlottesville- Virginia: Pediatric Psychology Press. Allison, J., Kilgallon, P. & Reilly, S. (1988). “The Children’ s Cottage. A Respr te Service for Children and Parents rn Crisis.” W Qarc- 3(5). 1- 12 Andrews, F. & Withey, S. (1976). ' ’ ' ° New York: Plenum Press. Belle, D. (1982). “Sodal Ties and Sodal Support.” In D. Belle (Ed) Limit] Winn. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications anins- J- &Kintz- B. (1968). QamtafionalfiandboaknflSt-afistics. Glenville, Illinois: Scott Foreman Company. Butler, T. 8: Friesen, B. (1988) WWII. Portland, Oregon. Portland Sate University. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 303 986). Carmines- E. & Zeller- R. (1979). WWW Beverly Hills: Sage Publication. Collette, N. 8: Lee, D. (1983). "The Impact of Support for Black Adolescent Mothers” Wanner-4(1). 127- 143. Collette, N. & Gregg, C. (1981). "Adolescent Mothers' Vulnerability to Stress.” WW 169(1)- 50-54. Conger, R. McCarty, J. Yang, R., Lahey, B., & Burgess, R. (1984) “Mother’s Age as a Predictor of Observed Maternal Behavior 1n Three Independent Samples of Families.” WW Earnilx- $5.- 411-423- D’Ercole, A. (1988). “Single Mothers: Stress, Coping, and Sodal Support. " W 16- 41-54. Fasan C &Gurry- S. (1981). Excennonalfihildranandfiasmtcfiam. W Cambridge, Massachusetts. (ERIC Docurrlent Reproduction Service No. 245 459). 81 Franz, J. (1980, J anuary/Februaruy) “Being There: A 24-Hour Emergency Crisis Care Center.” W, 7-10. French, J. (1990). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Frisbie, D. & Slater. K (1979). W Lansrng, Michigan: Family Support Project. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 219 919). Gladow, N. 8: Ray, M. (1986). “The Impact of Informal Support Systems on theWellBeingofLow Income Single Parents.” W35, 113-123. Glenn, N .& Weaver,C. (197 9). “A Note on Family Situation and Global Happiness.” Socialism. 51(3). 960-967. Hansenfeld, H., Murphy, 8., 8: Olson, K. (1981). “The Child Parent Drop-In Center: Community Based Primary Prevention.” W W. 2(3)- 155-158. Hayes, C. (1987) “Determinants of Adolescent Sexual Behavior and Dedsion Making.” In C. Hayes (Ed) . (pp. 95-139). Washington D.C.: National Academy. Heller, K. & Swindle, R. (1983). "Sodal Networks, Perceived Soda] Support- and Coping with Stress." In R. Felner, L. Jason, J. Moritsugu, & s. Faber (Eds.). WW (pp. 87-103). New York: Pergamon Press. Hogan, M., Bushler, C., & Robinson, B. (1983). “Single Parenting: Transitioning Alene”. In H. McCubbin & C. Figely (Eds) Streams] (pp. 116- 132). New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc. Itagliata, J. (1986). “Assessing the Impact of Respite Care Services: A Review of Outcome Evaluation Studies.” In C. Salisbury & J. Intagliata (Eds. ).. (pp. 263-288). Baltimore, Maryland. Paul H. Brooks. Joyce, K. 8; Singer, M. (1983). “Respite Care Services: An Evaluation of the Perception of Parents and Workers.” Wm, 44(9- 10): 270-274. Joyce, K., Singer, M., & Isralowitz, R. (1983). “Impact of Respite Care on Parents’ Perceptions of Quality of Life.” Wyn, 21(4): 153-156. . Lamanna, M. &Riedmann, A. (1988). ' ' theeunifiadnsflranae (3rd ed). Belmont California: Wadsworth Publishing Company. Lindblad-Goldberg, M., Dukes, J, & Lasley, J. (1988). ”Stress in Black, Low-Income Single-Parent Families. Normative and Dysfunctional Patterns " Americanianmalnmrthonmhiatrx- 55(1): 104-120. Lindblad-Goldberg, M., Dukes, J. ,& Lasley, J. (1985). "Sodal Support in Black, Low-Income Single-Parent Families: Normative and Dysfunctional Patterns." WWII!- 55(1) 42-58. Makosky, V. (1982). “ Sources of Stress: Events or Conditions?” In D. Belle (Ed.-) WWW (pp. 35-53). Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. Mandel], C. (1984).Dexelanins.1ndirddnalized£arent_2artidnatian Bowling Green, Ohio; Bowling Green State University, Department of Speda] Education. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 257 244). McAdoo, H. (1983). 'Sodetal Stress: The Black Family. In H. McCubbin 8: C. Figely (Eds. ). ' ' (pp. 178-187). New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc. McCubbin, H. 8: Patterson, J. (1983). “Family Transitions: Adaptation to Stress” In H. McCubbin & C. .Figely (Eds. ), (pp. 5-25). New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc. McLanahan, S. (1985). “Single Mothers and Psychological Well-Being: A Test of the Stress and Vulnerability Hypotheses” W Wealth- 5: 253-266. McLanahan, S. (1983). "Family Structure and Stress: A Longitudinal Comparison of Two-parent and Female-headed Families. Journal ofMarriaaeandtheEamilr. 45: 347-357. Miller, B. & Myers-Walls, J. (1983). “Parenthood: Stresses and Coping Strategies.” In H. McCubbin & C. Figely (Eds) (pp. 54-73). New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc. 83 \/ Nye, E. Carlson, J., 8: Garrett, G. (1970). “Family Size, Interaction, Afi‘ect, and Stress. llanmalnflliarriaseandthefiamrlx- 32: 216-222 Pascoe, J. & Earp, J. (1984). “The Effects of Mothers’ Sodal Support and Life Changes on the Stimulation of their children in the Home.” Americanianmalnflhrbliefiealth- 14(4): 358-360. wPascoe, J., Ialongo, N., Horn, W, Reinhart, M.A., & Perradatto, D. ( 1988) 35‘ “The Reliability and Validity of the Maternal Sodal Support Index. EarnibLMedidne 29(4): 271-276. <67 Pascoe, J., Chessare, J., Baugh, E. Urich, L. & Ialongo, N. (1987). “Help with Prenatal Household Tasks and newborn Birth Weight: Is there an Association?” alonmalntDelelamnentaLandBehaxiara] Pediatrics, 8(4): 207-212. Visas-roe, J. a French, J. (1990). “The Reliability and Validity of the Maternal Sodal Support Index for Primiparo us Mothers: A Brief Report.” Wane): 228-230pm \/Pascoe, J., Loda, F., Jefiiies, V., & Earp, J. (1981). "The Assodation Between Mothers' Sodal Support and Provision of Stimulation to their Children.” WWW W95- 2(1): 15-19. Pearlin, L. 8: Johnson, J. (1977). “Marital Status, Life-Strains, and Depression.” Ameficanfiadalodcalkxim- 42: 704-715. Peterson, E. (1990, September, 9). "Yes, It's Harder to be a Child." Detroit Freehan- p. 6H- Pittman, J. Wright, C., & Lloyd, S. (1989). "Predicting Parenting MIMIIY- WWW 19(2) 267-236. Rimmerman, A. (1989). “Provision of Respite Care for Children with Developmental Disabilities: Changes in Maternal Coping and Stress over Time.” WM]:- 21(2): 99-103. Salisbury,C (1986). “Generic Community Services as Sources of Respite.” In C Salisbury _& J Intagliata (Eds.) W ~ . ‘ = -= : . pp 195-216). Baltimore,Maryland: Paul H. Brooks Publishing Company. Straus, M. Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). W Garden City, New York. Anchor Press/Doubleday. Stenson, C. (1989). Pocateno. Idaho. Idaho State University. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 306 909). Subramanian, K. (1986). "Redudng Child Abuse through Respite Care Intervention." W MG): 501-509. Tellar, C., (personal communication, 1991). Michigan State Department of Soda] Services, Lansing, MI. Thoits, P. (1986). “Sodal Support as coping Assistance.” W hannselinaandfllinieallsxchalosx. 54(4): 416-423. flouliatos, J., Perlmutter, B., & Straus, M. (1990). ' MeasnrernenLIechnianes. Newbury Park: Sage Publications Unger, D. (1987). ' ' ° ' ' Columbia, South Carolina; South Carolina State Department of Health and Environmental Control. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 294 083). Unger, D. & Powell, D. (1980). “Supporting Families Under Stress: the Role of Sodal Networks.” Eamilyjelamms, 22: 566-574. Unger D. & Wandersman, L. (1988). "The Relation of Family and Partner Support to the Adjustment of Adolescent Mothers. Child W52: 1056-1060. Weinraub, M. & Wolf, B. (1983). "Efi'ects of Stress and soda] Supports on Mother-Child Interactions rn Single- and Two-Parent Families. Wayne-nt- 54:1297-1311 Weinstein, V. & Siegel, P. (1.988) ' Northridge, California. California Children' s Coundl. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 307 959). Wikler, L., Hanusa,D., 8r Stoychefi‘, J. (1986). "Home-Based Respite Care, the Child with Developmental Disabilities, and Family Stress. Some Theoretical and Pragmatic Aspects of Process Evaluation. "In C. Salisbury & J. Intagliata (Eds. )- (pp. 243-261). Baltimore, Maryland. Paul H. Brooks Publishing Company. Wilcox,B (1981). "Sodal Support, Life Stress, and Psychological Adjustment: A Test of the Buffering Hypothesis. American Wale-hologr- 9(4): 371-386. 85 Zuravin, S. (1988). "Child Maltreatment and Teenage First Births: A Relationship Mediated by Chronic Sododenographjc Stress?" Americaninmalnfflrthansxchiatnr. 53 1 91-103 Zur-Szpiro, S. & Longfellow, C. (1982). “Fathers’ Support to Mothers and Children.” InD Belle (Ed) Wand Dimension. (pp. 145-153). Beverly Hills; Sage Publications. Appendices Appendix A $ Appendix A Family Growth Center Quesionairre: FAMILY GROWTH CENTER QUESTIONNAIRE We would like to know how you learned about the Family Growth Centers (FGC). Please CIRCLE the appropriate number or FILL IN the blank for each question. 1. How did you first learn about the Family Growth Centers and its services? Circle all that apply. Friend Poster/brochure Newspaper (Which one) Referred by a caseworker/professional Church Presentation Other $999!”.N!‘ 2. What was the first service you used at the Family Growth Centers? Circle the number of your answer. 1. Drop-in child care Attended a support group Parent education classes Crisis Intervention Other 9‘99.” 87 3. How did you learn about the other services at FGC? Please drawalinefromeachserviceyouknow (ColumnA) tothe wayts) you found out about the service (column B). Ifyou do notknowofthese servicespleasegodirectiytoquestian4. Comm (2de Drop-in child care Referral by professional Recommended by a FGC stafl' member Support Groups Recommended by a FGC parent Read about it in a FGC poster/brochure Parent Education classes Other 4. When did you learn about the Family Growth Center's (FGC) drop-in child care? Circle the number of your answer. 1. When I first heard of the FGC 2. At my first visit to the FGC 3. After I had visited the FGC several times 4. Other 5. When did you first start using drop-in child care? Fill in the blanks. Month year 6. About how many times a month do you use drop-in child care at any center? Circle the correct number. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 7. At which center(s) do you use drop-in child care? Circle all that apple'- 1. Downtown Center 2. East Lansing Center 3. Mt. Hope Center 3 8. How do you plan to or do you use the time you have while your child is in drop-in child care? Circle all that apply. Rest ' Shap/family errands Visit with own fiiends Job interview Appointments Go to school Go to work Other 9°$9°9‘:“9’!°P 9. Have you ever attended a support group at FGC? Circle the numberofyouranswer. 1. Yes 2. No 10. Have you ever attended parenting classes at FGC? Circle the number-cumm- 1. Yes 2. No 11. Ifyouhaveonlyuseddmp-inchildcarcatFGC,couldyou please explain why you choose not to use the other services, support groups and parent education classes? $ We would like to know a little more about you and your family. Please CIRCIEtheappmpr-iatemnnherm-FIILIN theblankfioreachqnesflon. 12. Your age 13. 14. 15. 16. Your marital status 1. Never married 2. Married 3. Separated 4. Divorced 5. Widowed The number of years you have completed in school. Circle the 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 + How many children do you have? Circle the correct number. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10+ How many people are living with you at this time? (Please include spouse, children, relatives, and friends.) Circle the so 17. Pleaselistthefimtnamesofallthepeoplelivingwithyou, theirsextheiragewndthdrrelafionshiptoyou. Thank you for your time and cooperation in completing this questionnaire. Please continue to the next questionnaire, The Maternal Social Support Index (MSSI). Maternal Social Support Index: MATERNAL SOCIAL SUPPORT INDEX (MSSI) Please share with us the things you do in your home as a mother by answering the questions below. Please use the following scale and CIRCLE the answer you feel is m for you. 10. 11. If no car, can you get one in a few hours if needed? 12. How many relatives do you see once a week or more often ? o 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I Generally Generally Someone Else No one Do It Does It 1 2 3 Who fixes the meals? 1 2 Who does the grocery shopping? 1 2 Who lets your children know what is right or wrong? 1 2 Who fixes things around the house or apartment? 1 2 Who does the inside cleaning? 1 2 Who works outside around the house or apartment? 1 2 Who pays the bills? 1 2 Who takes your child to the doctor if he/she is sick? 1 2 Who sees to it that your children go to bed? 1 2 Who takes care of car problems No on short notice (if appropriate)? Car 2 8 3 Yes 9 Someone Else and I Do It 4 No 10+ 92 Please CIRCLE the answer that is me for you. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. Would you like to see relatives: More often Less often It's about right How many people can you count on in times of need? 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 + How many people would be able to take care of your children for several hours if needed; 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 + Do you have a boyfriend or husband? Yes No (If an, go to ll18) If yes, how happy are you in the way your boyfriend or husband lets you know what he feels or thinks? Very Very Happy Happy Unhappy Unhappy Are there adults, NOT including your boyfriend or husband, with whom you have regular talks? Ye s No (If no, go to M) If yes, think about the person you talk with the most. Are you happy with the talks that you have with this person? Very Very Happy Happy Unhappy Unhappy 33 For the next question, please use the following scale and CIRCLE the answer that is m for you: Don't Attend less Attend Attend more Belong than 1x a mo. 1x a mo. than 1x a mo. 1 2 3 4 H.999”? 21. How often do you attend meetings of the following groups? Religious (e. g. church) 1 Educational (e.g school, parent groups) 1 Social (e.g. bowling groups, scouting groups)1 Political (e.g. work for a local candidate) 1 Other: ______________________ 1 NNNNN mammal hubthnhfi Are you a member of any committee or do you have any other duties in any of your groups? Yes No How would you describe the amount of room your family has in your home? Use the scale below. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Not Enough More than Enough Room Enough Room Room 94 23. Please circle all of the things you have in your home. 599°:49’9‘P9’N!‘ H 9 Running water Electricity Heat Telephone Refrigerator Stove/oven Radio Television Washer Dryer PARENTING STRESS INDEX (Short Form) Richard R. Abidin University of Virginia Directions: in answering the following questions. please think about the child you are most concerned about. The questions on the following pages ask you to marl: an answer which best describes your feelings. While you may not find an answer which exactly states your feelings. please marl: the answer which comes closest to describing how you feel. Pluumrkthedegmtowhichyouagreewdhagreewiththefououdngsnmby circlingthenumberwhlchbestmatcheshowyoufeel. Uyouannotmpleasecirclen. .1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Agree Agree Not Sure Disagree Strongly Disagree . Example: Ienloy golngtothemoviee. (lfyouaunetimeeenjoygolngtothemovies. you would circle .2.) - 1 ® 3 g 5 Copyrighted 1990 - Abidin Not to be duplicated l . 2 3 ‘ g. 5 1. loftmhavethefeelingtluticannothandlethingsverywell. z Ifindmyseifgivingupmoreofmylifetomeetmychildren’sneedsthanl evcrcxpected. 3.1feeltrappedbymyresponsibilitiesasaparent. 4. Sincehavingthischildlhavebeenunabletodonewanddifferurtthings. S. Sincehavingachildlfeelthatlamalmcstneverabletodothingsthat! liketodo. 6. Iamunhappywiththelastpurchaseofclothinglmadeformyself. 7. Therearequiteafewthingsthatbothermeaboutmylife. 8. Havingachildhascaused moreproblenuthanlexpectedinmyrelationslup with my spouse (male/ female friend). 9. lfeelaloneand without friends. 10.Whenlgotoapartyiumallyexpectnottoenjoymyself. 11.1mnotasintereetedinpeopleaslusedtohe. 'lZIdon'tmjoythingsasiusedto. l3. Mychildrarelydomthinpformethatmakemefeelgood. 14. Mosttimeslfeeithatmychilddoeenotlikemeanddoesnotwanttobe doeetome. 15. Mychildsmileeatmenmdtlesthanlexpected. l6. Whenldothingsformycluldlgetthefedingthatmyeffortsarenot apprschtedveryaudt. 17. Whenphyhpmydtilddomn'toftengiggleorlaugh. 18. Mydtilddoon'tseemtoleamasquicklyasmcstdtildren. 19.Mydulddoesn'tseemtosmileunnrchasmostd1ildren 20. Mycluldisnotabletodoasmchaslexpected. l l 21.1thkesalongdnuanditisveryhardformychildtogetusedtonewthings. i Copyrightedlm-Afldin Nottoheduplicaud 2345 . 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Agree Agree Not Sure Dingree Smgly Disagree 22- ”RUIN“!!! 1. notverygoodatbeingaparent. 2. a person who has some trouble being a parent. 3. an average parent. 1 2 3 4 5 . 4. a better than average parent. 5. a very good parent. 2. Iexpectedtohavecioserandwarmerfeelingsformychildthanldoandthis bothers me. 1 2 3 4 S 24. Sometimesmychilddoestlungsthatbothermejusttobemean. 1 2 3 4 5 econ: 25. Mychildseemstoayorhissmoreoftenthanmostchildren 1 2 3 4 s 26. My child generally wakes up in a bad mood. 1 2 3 4 S 27.1feelthatmychildisverymoodyandeasilyupeet. 1 2 3 4 5 28. My child does a few things which bother me a great deal. 1 2 3 4 5 29. My child reacts very strongly when something happens that my child doesn't like. 1 2 3 4 5 30. Mychildgetsupeeteasilyoverthesmallestthing. 1 2 3 4 5 31. My child's sleeping or eating schedule was much harder to establish than i expected 1 2 3 4 s 32.1havefoundthatgettingmychildtodoeomethingorstopdoing mhinsis 1. muchharderthanl 2. somewhatharderthanlexpected. 3. aboutashardaslexpected. 1 2 3 4 s 4. somewhatmsierthanl 5. muchmsierthanlexpected. 33. lhinkarefullyandcountthemmberoftlfingswhichymrdulddoestlut botheryou. Formmple: dawdlarehuestolistenmveractivncries. interrupufighnwhinethc. Pleasedrdethenumberwhichinciudesthe mimberofthingsymianmtsd. 1.104- 2.” 3.67 4.4-5 5.1-3 1 2 3 4 5- 34.1'herearesomethingsmychilddoesthatreallybothermealot. 1 2 3 4 s 35. Mychildnirnedouttobemoreofaproblemthanlhadexpected. 1 2 3 4 5 36. Mychildmakesmoredemandsonmethanmostchildren. 1 2 3 4 s Copyrighud 1990-am Nottobeduplicand “NW" Appendix B $ Appendix B Letter from Director to participants: Family Growth Center A Program of Child Abuse Prevention Services January 28, 1991 Dear Parents: Maria Townsend is a friend of the Family Growth Center. She is Helping us learn more about the needs of parents who use the center. Thank you for taking part in this study. It will help the Family Growth Center provide a better service to you. YOUR ANSWERS ARE TOTALLY CONFIDENTIAL. THE FGC STAFF WILL NEVER SEE TEI-IM. Please be as frank as possible, so that Maria’s research can give us a true picture of the needs of families who use the center. We so appreciate your help with this, that we want to thank you in a special way. When you complete the forms, bring them back to the desk. M11 -:u ttt o It-:‘::yttt ': ‘ s 5‘ t t t o ‘l Thank you again for participating in this project. And please don’t forget to ask at the desk for a Child Care Coupon. Sincerely, Merry Stanford Director Family Strengthening Programs Letter to participants: Dear Mother: We would like you to tell us how you use the Family Growth Centers (FGC). ’ We would also like to know if you find being a mother stressful and how much other people help you in your role as a mother. The results of this study will be used by the staff of FGC to better serve your needs as a parent and by me to complete a requirement for my college classes. The questionnaire packet, which includes a green consent form, a blue release form, three questionnaires, and a 3x5 card, will only take 20 to 35 minutes to complete. All the materials that you complete will be identified by an identification number only. Your name will not be linked to your answers on any of the materials. This ensures that your answers will be _ totally confidential. To participate in this study, you must read and sign both the green consent form and the blue release form found in your questionnairepacket. IN NO WAY ARE YOU REQUIRED TO PARTICIPATE IN THIS STUDY, NOR WILL NOT PARTICIPATING AFFECT YOUR BEING ABLE TO USE THE SERVICES. YOU MAY CHOOSE TO ANSWER ONLY THOSE QUESTIONS YOU FEEL COMFORTABLE ANSWERING. IF YOU DO NOT WISH TO PARTICIPATE IN ANY WAY, PLEASE RETURN YOUR QUESTIONNAIRE PACKET. I will be happy to answer any questions you may have about this study. Please feel free to write to me at the address below or call me at (517) 351- 7617. If you wish to receive a summary of the results of this study, please check the box on the 3x5 card. Thank you for your time and assistance. Sincerely Maria Zeglen Townsend Institute for Child, Youth & Families Paolucci Building, Unit 2 Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48823 100 Consent Form: CONSENT FORM 1. I, the undersigned, understand that the purpose of this study is to understand the effects of the stress and support I am experiencing as a mother on my participation in drop-in child care offered at the Family Growth Centers. 2. My participation in this study will involve my completion of a packet of questionnaires, which will take approximately 20 to 35 minutes. 3. I understand that my answers will in no way be linked to my name and that they are confidential. 4. I understand that I can choose not to participate in this study at any time and this decision will not afi‘ect my being able to use the services at the Family Growth Centers. 5. I understand that I can obtain a summary of the study's findings by checking the box on the 3x5 card in the questionnaire packet. 6. I understand that by participating in this study, I will receive a reserved care coupon which entitles me to one session of reserved grop-in child care for all of my children, ages six weeks through ve years. ' 7. If I have any questions or concerns about my participation in this study, I know that I am encouraged to contact Mrs. Maria Zeglen Townsend at 351-7617 to discuss my concerns. V ' 8. I have read the above and agree to participate in this study. A. I, the researcher, guarantee confidentiality to the person who signed the form. Your name will not be linked to any of your answers on the questionnaires. I will be happy to address any questions or concerns you may have about the questionnaires and your participation in this study. Maria Zeglen Townsend Graduate Student 101 Release Form: RELEASE FORM I give my permission to the Family Growth Centers to release general information from the forms that I have completed for the Center. This will include information such as, my age at the birth of my first child, income, and the number of times I have used drop-in child care for my children to the following person: Maria Zeglen Townsend Institute for Child, Youth 8: Families Paolucci Building, Unit 2 Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824 “‘1. This permission will end as of December 31, 1991. Signature Date Reserved Care Coupon & 3x5 Card: ReservedCamCoupon The reserved care coupon was created as an incentive to encourage participation in the study. It will be given to the mothers after they have completed the questionnaires. The coupon will be copied onto bright, heavy paper. The following will appear on the front of the coupon: N Reserved care for your children, ages six weeks throum years, for one session of care at the Family Growth Center of your choice. You must call before 8:15 AM to reserve care for the mornings session or before 12:45 PM to reserve care for the afternoon session. Spaces may be limiwd on any given day. 484-2610 E : June 1991 3x5 Card The 3x5 card placed in the questionnaire envelope will allow the researcher to track the mother's participation in the parent education classes or support groups and still ensure the mother's confidentiality. The 3x5 card will have the following information on it. Ember __ Please clearly print your first and last name: [ ] Please check the box if you would like a ofthis stud 's fin ' . f . Checklist for outside of envelope: STOP Before you hand in your completed questionnaires at the desk, please make sure you did these things. 1. Did you mad the green CONSENT form and sign your name at the bottom? Did you mad the blue RELEASE form and sign your we at the bottom? Have you completed the: ___FGC Questionnaire, Maternal Social Support Index, and Parenting Stress Index? Have you printed your name on the 3x5 card and checked the box if you would like to receive a summary report of this study's findings? Please man fiom the envelope the: 3x5 card __ Signed green CONSENT form __ Signed blue RELEASE form and seal the envelope shut. Slam place in the locked, wooden box on or near the desk e: __Completed 3x5 card __Signed green, CONSENT form __Signed blue, RELEASE form Please place your sealed envelope in the designated box next to the locked, wooden box. Don't forget to ask at the desk for your Reserved Drop-in Child Care Coupon. Thank oykou for your time and careful consideration in completing this packet questionnaires. 104 Pretest Questionairre: mmasr QUESTIONNAIRE ' We would like to know how you would rate the questionnaire packet you just completed. Please read the questions carefully and CIRCLE the answer that best describes your experience in completing the questionnaire packet. 1. Was the consent form understandable and clear? very somewhat average somewhat very clear clear unclear unclear 2. Was the release form understandable and clear? very somewhat average somewhat very clear clear unclear unclear 3. How easy were the instructions to follow for the Demographic Questionnaire ? very somewhat average somewhat very easy easy difl‘icult diflicult 4. Was the Demographic Questionnaire understandable and clear? very somewhat average somewhat very clear clear unclear unclear 5. How easy were the instructions to follow for the Parenting Stress Index ? very somewhat average somewhat very easy easy dimcult dificult 105 6. Was the Parenting Stress Index understandable and clear? very somewhat average somewhat very clear clear unclear unclear 7. How easy were the instructions to follow for the Maternal Social Support Index ? very somewhat average somewhat very easy easy diflicult difiicult 8. Was the Maternal Social Support Index understandable and clear? very somewhat average somewhat very clear clear unclear unclear 9. Do you have any additional comments that may help us to make the questionnaire packet more understandable and easy to complete? 10. Was the checklist on the envelope helpful? very somewhat average somewhat very helpful helpful useless useless Poster: If you are not attending our parent classes or support groups and are interested in one guaranteered session of care for your child, Please see the receptionist or other staff members. Flyer: Attention Mothers A reserved child care coupon is being offered to all participants of a research study. The study will examine the influences of stress and social support on mothers who use drop-in child care offered by the Family Growth Centers. To participate in the study, you must be using drop- in child care at the downtown, East Lansing, or Mt. Hope centers. Please ask the receptionist or other staff members for more information. The study is being conducted by Mrs. Maria Zeglen Townsend, a graduate student at Michigan State University, (who can . be reached at 351-7617) in cooperation with the staff of the Mt. Hope and downtown Family Growth Centers. Greeting card to mothers: mm mm 'm mm aims Mum n at m u no mu use 5? ueieess sue sq sesame am pm shell us so; secede cusses q ndi nous mA Ml ounce use mess: e sauces use not 'iauiut cm ili animals! us inm- want new no 'm-Iul m w M Mama: Nut ti m Jams «we Ilium an he would: ti union cm 'ma Inna lit-“JP In on one 040mm 1' hue-l Mud «um-01mm»! alumna-m CHIN“ Print 3““ I991 0 I 93-99? ”001W UUOUd Ind oo=v - M 0m Jams mum in: annual «Id oo=v - «It can mm «mm limo-M an era l - m on mm mil in 0mm «Id oo=tv - M can mm mu in 0mm ”N33 J'A [M96 You and your children, ages six weeks through five users, are invited to try the Family Growth Centers' drop-in child care. Euchris Letter: MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY mammalian-cam MW'MM'Cfl-i“ ”MOMMA?!“ November 12, 1990 RE: IRE! 90-472 Maria Zeglen Townsend Fanily and Child Ecology Unit 2, Paolucci bldg. RE: IRE FACTORS OE HATERNAL STRESS AND SOCIAL SUPPORT RELATED TO HOTEERS' PAliICIPATIOR IN PARENT PROGRAMS AMONG USERS OE RESPITE CHILD CARE. IRE 90-472 Dear as. Townsend: the above project is axe-pt free full 081188 review. The proposed research protocol has been reviewed by another connittee eenber. The rights and welfare of hunen subjects appear to be protected and you have approval to conduct the research. You are relinded that 0611!! approval is valid for one calendar year. If you plan to continue this project beyond one year. please sake provisions for obtaining appropriate UCIIIS approval one nonth prior to hove-bar 4, 1991. Any changes in procedures involving hunan subjects nust be reviewed by ucrxss prior to initiation of the change. OCIIIS nuet also be notified pronptly of any problees (unexpected side effects. couplainte. etc.) involving hunan subjects during the course of the work. thank you for bringing this project to ny attention. If I can be of any future help. please do not hesitate to let us know. Sincerely, < .l 2122'.“ o'me 3. Wright. Chair. scars: DIV/doc cc: Dr. Jeanne Brown NSU'IQMWWW ”’11111111111313