LIBRARY Michigan State L University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove thie checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before due due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE iaélfl'ms, fl . ll l fl_ JD ll D1 if— MSU I. An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity institution cMMpma-M THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PARENTAL PERCEPTION OF CHILD BEHAVIOR AND PARENTAL CHILDHOOD MEMORIES: A COMPARISON OF MOTHERS OF CONDUCT-DISTURBED AND NON-CONDUCT-DISTURBED BOYS By Jerry Stuart Adams A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1992 ABSTRACT THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PARENTAL PERCEPTION OF CHILD BEHAVIOR AND PARENTAL CHILDHOOD MEMORIES: A COMPARISON OF MOTHERS OF CONDUCT-DISTURBED AND NON-CONDUCT-DISTURBED BOYS By Jerry Stuart Adams The purpose of this study was to establish a connection between a mother’s childhood experiences and her current parenting. The study proposed that the mother’s perceptual style is the result of an affective-cognitive model regarding parent-child interactions and this mental model is based largely on the mother’s internal representation of her own childhood relationships. The participants were 71 mothers of conduct-disturbed boys (MCDB) and 62 mothers of normal boys (MNB). The mothers completed a) the Child Behavior Scenes, an instrument designed to assess their deviancy-classification schema (i.e., the tracking and labelling of positive, negative and neutral child behaviors), b) the Child Behavior Rating Scales, designed to obtain the mothers’ judgments regarding the degree of aversiveness and the degree of desirability of specific child behaviors, and c) the Adult Attachment Questionnaire (AAQ), a questionnaire format of the Berkeley Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan & Main (1985). The results indicated that the MCDB are overly inclusive in the classification of deviancy and are more "sensitized" and "reactive" to child misbehavior. The were no differences in terms of the classification of prosocial behaviors. While the two groups of mothers did not differ in the content of their childhood memories, there were significant differences in the organizational qualities of these recollections. The MCDB childhood memories were significantly more fragmented and disorganized, and lacking in specific detail. The MCDB were significantly more "unresolved" and less individuated from their childhood relationships. Overall, the psychopathology of the child was related to the mother’s affective-cognitive processing, synthesis, and integration of her own childhood experiences but not the actual experiences in—and-of-themselves. While the organization of the childhood memories did not account for the MCDB vigilance towards deviancy, there were some significant relationships between the organization of the mother’s recollections and her current perceptual style. The study offers support for the construct of an internal mental model bridging past experiences with current functioning. Also, the AAQ provided a good alternative to the full-length interview. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Robert Caldwell, chairperson, Dr. Hiram Fitzgerald, Dr. Albert Aniskiewicz and Dr. Ellen Strommen for serving as members of my dissertation committee. I would like to express my special appreciation to Dr. Caldwell for his conscientious guidance, support, and valuable suggestions. I am very thankful to Dr. Caldwell for his patient and continued encouragement of my own autonomy and competency. As a result of his conduct and persistent respect for my integrity, the completion of this research resulted in fostering my own growth and development intellectually, professionally and emotionally. I would like to acknowledge the cooperation and commitment of the participating mothers and the cooperation of the participating school districts. I would like to thank the raters for their input and effective scoring of the AAQ, and the clinicians that helped in the development of the Child Behavior Scenes. Relatedly, an important thank you to Dr. Mary Main for granting permission to use the Adult Classification System. I would like to express my gratitude to Suzy Pavick for her expert assistance in getting the final draft of this manuscript printed and to the graduate school. A special thank you to Jim McComb for his support and assistance with the statistics. iv t.p Finally, I am very appreciative of the endless support and encouragement offered by my friends, especially Chet, Dori and Bella. A special thank you to Ann Wagner for her fi'iendship, kindness, and the many hours of phone consultations, and to Ellen Ives, a cherished friend, a continual source of encouragement and motivation, and a very thorough reader of the final draft. A very special heartfelt thank you to Dr. Sam Plyler. LE F NTENT PAGE CHAPTER 1--INTRODUCTION .................... 1 Conduct Disorders ........................ 7 Classification ........................ 8 Coercion Theory ..................... 12 Irnpressionistic Overview ................ 22 Discipline and a Review of the Empirical Literature on Family Interactions .......... 24 Parental Cognitive Variables .................. 39 Parental Perception and Labelling of Child Behavior ..................... 43 Parental Categorization of Deviant, Neutral and Prosocial Behaviors ........... 47 The Perspective of the Present Study ............. 53 Attachment Theory .................... 60 Parental Recollections .................. 67 Summary .......................... 73 Hypotheses ............................. 75 Group I - Maternal Perception of Child Behavior . 76 Group II - Maternal Childhood Recollections . . . . 79 Group III - Relationship Between Maternal Perception and Childhood Recollections ...... 81 CHAPTER 2-—METHODS ........................ 83 Participants ............................. 83 Mothers of Conduct Disturbed Boys ......... 84 Mothers of Normal Boys ................ 85 Demographic Variables ................. 88 Participant Selection ................... 90 Instruments ............................. 91 Child Behavior Checklist ................ 92 Family Information Sheet ................ 93 Family Togethemess Scale ............... 93 Child Behavior Scenes .................. 94 Child Behavior Rating Scales ............. 98 Preliminary Questions ................. 100 Adult Attachment Questionnaire ........... 101 vi Training ......................... 111 Determination of Inter-Rater Reliability ...... 112 Procedure ............................. 114 CHAPTER 3--RESULTS ........................ 118 Group I - Maternal Perception of Child Behavior . . . . 118 Group II - Maternal Childhood Recollections ....... 128 Group III - Relationship Between Maternal Perception and Childhood Recollections ..... 133 Sub-Sample Analysis ...................... 135 CHAPTER 4--DISCUSSION ..................... 151 Maternal Perception ....................... 153 Maternal Recollections ..................... 171 Maternal Recollections and Perceptual Style ........ 185 Summary and Implications .................. 200 APPENDD( A - Letter Distributed to Mothers ........... 205 APPENDIX B - Phone Interview and Fact Sheet .......... 206 APPENDDI C - Consent Letter .................... 208 APPENDIX D - Feedback to Participants .............. 211 APPENDDI E - Family Information Sheet ............. 212 APPENDIX F - Family Togethemess Questionnaire ....... 213 APPENDDI G - Instructions for the Child Behavior Scenes . . . 214 APPENDIX H - The Experts’ Final Determination of the Positive (P), Negative (N), or Neutral (Blank) Value for Each Behavior in the Child Behavior Scenes ........... 216 APPENDIX I - Child Behavior Rating Scales I/II ......... 228 APPENDIX J - Adult Attachment Questionnaire .......... 240 References ................................. 257 vii 9. TABLES . Principal Characteristics of Unsocialized Aggressive and Socialized Aggressive Conduct Disorder as Derived from Multivariate Statistical Studies ............................. Child Behavior Checklist Alphas, Group Means, and Group Comparisons ........ . The List of Behaviors That Are Represented on the Extemalizing Scales of the CBCL ........ . Demographic Variables, Group Means and Group Comparisons .................. . Percentage of Agreement Between Raters and for Scales During Rater Training .......... The Percentage of Inter-Rater Agreement-Within-One for Raters and Scales .................... Hypothesis 1: Are MCDB Overly Inclusive in the Categorizationof Deviancy and Overly Exclusive in Their Categorization of Prosocial Behavior? . . . . Hypothesis 2: Are MNB Overly Inclusive in the Categorizationof Prosocial Behaviors and Exclusive in Their Categorization of Deviancy? .......... Intercorrelations Between the Perceptual Variables . . . . 10. Relationship Between Enmeshment and the Mother’s Deviancy - Classification Schema ....... 11. Comparisons Between the MCDB and the MNB on the Content of Their Childhood Recollections . . . 12. Comparisons Between the MCDB and the MNB on the Affective-Cognitive Organizational Properties of Their Childhood Recollections ............. viii PAGE ...86 ...87 ...89 .. 125 .. 126 .. 130 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21 22. Intercorrelations Between the Scales of the AAQ ....... Relationship Between the Affective-Cognitive Organization of Childhood Memories and the Mothers’ Deviancy Categorization Processes .............. Comparison of the Deviancy Categorization Process Between MCDB and MNB in the Sub-Sample . . Intercorrelations Between the Perceptual Variables for the Sub-Sample ........................ Comparisons for the Sub-Sample Between the MCDB and the MNB on the Content of Their Childhood Recollections Comparisons Between the Sub-Sample of MCDB and the MNB on the Affective-Cognitive Organizational Properties of Their Childhood Recollections ........ Relationship Between the Affective-Cognitive Organization of Childhood Memories and the Mother’s Deviancy Categorization Processes in the Sub-Sample ........ Partial Correlation Coefficients Between the Perceptual Variables of the CBS With the Effects of Responsivity Held Constant .................. . Intercorrelations Between the Childhood Experiences with the Mother and the Affective- Cognitive Organizational Scales of the AAQ ........ Intercorrelations Between the Childhood Experiences with the Father and the Affective- 132 134 140 144 147 179 Cognitive Organizational Scales of the AAQ ........ 180 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION It is a generally held belief in our culture that parents’ childrearing practices will have a direct effect on their offsprings’ development. New parents frequently refer to books like Dr. Spock’s for advice and suggestions regarding the correct childrearing strategy, as if this will help to insure a "positive" outcome. This generally held belief also has a tradition within the social sciences and is exemplified by Baumrind’s (1971) now classic study which demonstrates that specific parental attitudes and behaviors can have differential effects, either salutary or detrimental, on the child’s emotional and social development. Currently, however, there is general agreement that the parental influences are not unidirectional, but rather reciprocal with the child also having an influence on the parent (Bell, 1979). Within this more SOphisticated conceptual schema there is a whole array of potentially Significant family interactions, including not only parent to child and child to parent, but also marital interactions and those between siblings. Clearly, most contemporary theories on child/adolescent development now emphasize these family interactions as important factors contributing to the development of personality and psychopathology. This emphasis on family interactions is exemplified in the research literature on childrens’ conduct disorders. The association of specific family variables (e.g., 1 "an5 ti... .,| .-,-. 11 ‘ 43., .: 'hv- .. if . fi“. y Par-s. ‘ .... . \ W. . do "' .‘_.}: \. P's ‘\ T; "K \ p.‘ e 1.; I'\ P r. .. ‘ ~."|1 5“ c “I 0v 3‘ 2 family problem solving, discipline and/or parental monitoring) with the occurrence of a conduct disorder is now a well established fact consistently supported by recent comprehensive reviews of the literature (Hetherington & Martin, 1986; Patterson, 1982; Rutter & Garmezy, 1983; Rutter & Giller, 1983; Snyder & Patterson, 1987). One of the key factors that distinguishes families of children with a conduct disorder from "normal" families appears to be differences in parental styles or characteristics (i.e. , the differing ways in which a parent engages in or performs their parental duties/ responsibilities). ”There is abundant evidence to support a growing consensus that antisocial children have parents who lack family management skills" (Patterson, 1986, p.432). Thus, there are certain apparent deficiencies in the parenting style (e. g. , inept discipline practices) that are associated with the occurrence of a conduct disorder. It is essential to point out, however, that the behavioral family therapy literature has been criticized as over-emphasizing the position that the basic processes contributing to the child’s conduct disturbance is a parenting skills deficit (Griest & Wells, 1983). Griest and Wells (1983) stated that only recently has the ”treatment vehicle” (i.e. , the parent) been identified as potentially requiring intervention in areas other than family management skills. "In this expanded model it is hypothesized that the parent’s cognitive, psychological, marital and/or social adjustment is directly or indirectly related to conduct disorders in children" (p.38). Whether the focus is on inept family management and/or parental cognitive factors, the fact that there are a variety of family variables and specifically parental .‘up A n‘. t." . ”t; a in... Liz-.2. “’30" .L. g 32‘ .I . c’i- 3 characteristics that differentiate parents with normal children from the parents of children with a conduct disorder raises two interesting and related issues. First is the issue that Rutter and Giller (1983) referred to as "cause correlate or consequence? " This question raises interpretation issues regarding causality and the meaning of the associations between parental/family variables and disturbances of conduct. Is the parents’ behavior a direct cause for the conduct disturbance? Are the associations caused by some other third factor, for example, genetic, neighborhood, or social class? Or, as suggested by Patterson (1986b), is the parental behavior a response or consequence of the child’s deviancy (a notion supported by recent research, e. g., Anderson, Lytton & Romney, 1986)? These are difficult questions to resolve and, as Rutter and Giller (1983) pointed out, it is not always possible to determine causality or its direction. If, however, a relevant parental factor can be shown to have anmdated the child’s disturbance, then this supports an interpretation regarding causality. Overall, Rutter and Giller (1983) concluded that ”it seems likely that the causal influences are bi-directional but that, at least with some of the family variables, the preponderant effect is likely to be from parent to child" (p. 190). Relatedly, the second issue is raised within a broader context by Belsky (1984): By tradition, students of socialization have directed their primary energies toward understanding processes whereby parents’ childrearing strategies and behaviors shape and influence their offsprings’ development. It is of interest to learn that, while great effort has been expended studying the characteristics .I as 5 Jul -.....,, ‘tée. Finn. ~..._ .\ \ \ , K \l'l- \‘U ‘ 4 and consequences of parenting, much less attention has been devoted to studying why parents parent the way they do--beyond, of course, social-class and cross-cultural comparisons and investigations examining the effect of the child on parenting behavior. (p.83) In other words, why do parents of children with a conduct disorder have characteristics that differentiate them from " normal " parents? What are the determinants of the parental functioning or, more specifically the parental dysfunction? The present study is designed to shed some light on these important questions. This study will examine the relationship between parental cognitive variables and the child’s conduct disturbance. As the literature indicates, the parents of conduct disordered children do exhibit a variety of deficiencies in family management, especially discipline. These deficiencies are consistently associated with the child’s disturbed behavior. The present Study will expand this notion of inept family management, as suggested by Griest and Wells ( 1983), and examine whether there are underlying parental cognitive factors that account, in part, for the child’s mismanagement. This study is interested in the parental cognitive factors that may determine the parent’s perception of their child’s behavior. It is only common sense that the ways in which the parent perceives the child’s behavior will color or bias a wide variety 0f parent-child interactions. This is certainly true in regards to disciplinary interactions. The ways in which a parent defines the child’s behavior will largely I11. S-J LI tl' 5 determine the parental response. The parent’s classification of a behavior as deviant, as opposed to neutral or prosocial, is a critical initial step in the disciplinary process. It is the position of the present study that parents may contribute directly to the development of antisocial behavior by failing to adequately and consistently label, track, and ”consequate" deviant behaviors, as well as by not responding positively to prosocial behaviors. The present study will examine the parents’ deviancy- categorization processes (Patterson, 1986b). Hence, this study is designed to examine whether the parents of conduct disturbed children exhibit a different perceptual style, or rather, a different cognitive schema for categorizing behaviors as deviant, neutral, and/or prosocial, as compared to the parents of normal children. Additionally, this study will examine why it is that these parents behave as they do. In other words, what are the possible determinants for the parents’ perceptual style? Belsky (1984) provides a model for examining the determinants of parenting and provides fOr three general sources of influence on parental functioning: 1) the parent’s own developmental history and psychological resources; 2) the child’s characteristics and 3) the contextual sources of stress and support. The present study is concerned with the first factor, and will assess whether the quality of the parents’ recollections regarding their own childhood is related to both their current perceptual style and the psychopathology of their children. The position of the present study is that the parents’ perceptual style is the result of an affective-cognitive model regarding parent-child interactions. This mental (or representational) model is based largely on the parent’s own childhood 6 relationships, and therefore, the ways in which the parents recollect their own childhood experiences should exhibit the structural/organizational properties of this mental model. It is proposed that parents of conduct disturbed children have a mental model that is defensive in its orientation (i.e. , it is organized to distort and exclude certain classes of information). The present study proposes that this defensively oriented affective-cognitive model will be related to the parents’ perceptual style, as evidenced by their deviancy-categorization schema. The review of the literature is organized into three main sections. The first section on Conduct Disorders is designed to provide an understanding of the family characteristics and parental variables that differentiate families with a conduct disturbed child from normal families. The empirical literature is reviewed with a focus on those component behaviors that are likely to be involved in disciplinary interactions. The second section on Parental Cognitive Factors will suggest that these observed differences in family interactions can, in part, be accounted for by differences in parental cognitive processes. The research on both the parental perception of child behavior and the parents’ deviancy-categorization schema will be reviewed. The final section, Perspective of the Present Study, summarizes the previous two sections and develops the conceptual foundation/position for this study. Specifically, this section offers a detailed accounting to the following question: How is it that the quality of the parents’ recollections regarding their own childhood can be related to current caregiving behaviors? 1.5:" . , A ...uu. n; e . W I ‘ .1 y fi“:~e.. "We“ ‘ 1:11“ I i -Q:€T I ‘. \. \I ‘h. O ‘-1. . . .\. \ . a up WIS This section will review some of the current thinking and research on Conduct Disorders. The focus will be on the variety of parental characteristics and family interaction patterns that characterize families with conduct disordered children. There will be an emphasis on those parent-child interactions that are involved in the effective implementation of discipline. This section will be divided into four sub-sections: 1. Initially, it will be helpful to briefly review the literature on the classification of Conduct Disorders and provide some sense of the scope of the behavioral disturbance that defines this diagnostic category. 2. It will also be useful to provide a theoretical framework for understanding the family processes involved in the development and maintenance of a conduct disturbance. This conceptual framework will be provided by reviewing the extensive work on Coercion Theory (Patterson, 1982). 3. An impressionistic overview of the research findings on the relationship between family factors and conduct disorders will be presented. 4. This final sub-section commences by presenting a conceptual understanding of discipline, including those component behaviors involved in the parents’ disciplinary practices. With an emphasis on these component behaviors, a review of the empirical literature is undertaken, examining the behavioral differences between family interactions in families with a conduct disturbed child as compared to "normal" families. This sub-section will conclude with a review of the research specifically 8 focused on the relationship between parental " family management skills" , especially discipline, and conduct disturbance. Classification. Disturbances of conduct represent a major dimension of child/adolescent psychopathology. Rutter and Garrnezy (1983) reported that general population studies and studies of clinic samples "have been consistent in showing that disturbances of conduct (as indicated by fighting, temper tantrums, defiance, destructiveness, and generally uncooperative or inconsiderate behavior) constitute one of the most frequent causes of concern regarding children’s behavior" (p. 814). Factor analytic studies (as reviewed in Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1978; Quay, 1979, 1986a) have consistently shown that disturbances of conduct constitute one of the main dimensions of behavior during childhood and adolescence. In fact, Quay (1986b) reported that conduct disorders are "the most prevalent" form of childhood and adolescent psychopathology. "No other disorder of childhood and adolescence is so widespread and disruptive of the lives of those who suffer it and of the lives of others, and thus deserves continued research investigation" (p.64). There does appear to be general agreement that disturbances of conduct are heterogeneous. While clinical classification systems have a variety of sub-divisions, for example DSM-III has four subtypes of conduct disorders, factor analytic studies (e.g., Quay 1979, 1986a) have suggested that the evidence is very strong for the existence of two forms of conduct disorders; namely, an Unsocialized Aggressive and a Socialized Aggressive pattern. The results of Quay’s (1986a) multivariate statistical analysis of 61 studies in which descriptors of behavior have been analyzed and factor a i-“ Table 1 PM u. -11.. .' 'I f n 0 tr A" ‘ iv -. (13 i ' ed Are Sive r riv fr m M l'v ' 'tisi ie " Wm ' ' A 8 iv Fighting, hitting,assaultive (42)" Has "bad" companions (10) Disobedient, defiant (40) Truant from school (8) Temper tantrums (39) Truant from home (6) Destructiveness (35) Steals in company with others (6) Irnpertinent, "smart", impudent (31) Belongs to a gang (6) Uncooperative, resistant, Is loyal to delinquent friends (5) inconsiderate, stubborn (28) Stays out late at night (5) Attention seeking, "show-off" (26) Lies, Cheats (2) Dominates, bullies,threatens (26) Steals at home (2) Disruptive, interrupts, disturbs others (26) Boisterous, noisy (25) Irritability, "blows up" easily (23) Negative, refuses directions (22) Restless (21) Untrustworthy, dishonest, lies (20) Hyperactivity (18) 'The analysis is based on 61 studies spanning almost 40 years in which descriptors of behavior have been analyzed and factor loadings reported. (Quay, 1986a, p.12) l"Total number of studies in which characteristic was found associated with this dimension is in parentheses. 10 loadings reported are presented in Table l. The dimension labeled Unsocialized Aggressive Conduct Disorder is most frequently associated with behaviors that are generally considered aggressive, disruptive, and noncompliant. In the Socialized Aggressive Conduct Disorder, involvement with peers in illegal or at least norm- violating behavior is central. Further support for this position is provided by Loeber and Schmaling (1985b). They attempted to identify patterns of antisocial child behavior, reviewing 28 factor and cluster analyses of child behavior assessment instruments covering over 10,000 children. They concluded that antisocial behavior can be conceptualized as unidimensional and bipolar. One end of the dimension was characterized by overt or confrontive behaviors, such as fighting and other forms of aggressiveness. The other pole was characterized by covert or clandestine antisocial behaviors, such as alcohol and/or drug use, truancy, and theft. While there is statistical support for distinguishing between these two patterns of conduct disorders, Rutter and Gannezy (1983) suggested that the major justification for the differentiation between unsocialized/overt and socialized/covert patterns of deviant behavior comes from evidence showing that they differ in terms of family background and of outcome. Patterson’s (1982) distinction between "social aggressors" and "stealers" is conceptually consistent, as he provided preliminary evidence which suggests that there are different developmental and family processes that underlie the emergence of overt/confrontive antisocial behavior compared to covert/concealed antisocial behavior. Relatedly, Loeber and Schmaling’s (1985a) J. b\§ 1;: 5 pl '6 “H l..‘ ll findings offer further support for the utility of differentiating between an overt and a covert classification which is based on a typology of antisocial behavior, as the data does reveal some significant differences in terms of family processes, specifically parental characteristics. It is essential that research on conduct disorders take into account these potentially distinct patterns of antisocial behavior and clearly specify the relevant dimensions of the population under study. Not only will this aid in progressing the state of the art in terms of reliability, generalization, and replicability of results, but also, if covert and overt antisocial behavior do represent developmentally distinct patterns, then greater knowledge of these patterns will facilitate prediction, prevention, and treatment (Loeber & Schmaling, 1985b). The present study is focused on the family processes involved in the Unsocialized Aggressive Conduct Disorder, referred to by Patterson (1982) as "overt antisocial" or "Social Aggressors". Families of "overt antisocial" children have been characterized as "enmeshed" , with the parents exhibiting an overinclusiveness in their definition of problematic behaviors (Snyder & Patterson, 1987). Thus, the interactions that characterize these families easily lend themselves to specific hypotheses regarding the cognitive variables involved in the parent’s deviancy- categorization processes. With this specific sub-category as a focus, this literature review will make every attempt to distinguish which type of Conduct Disorder is being investigated. As Quay (1986b) pointed out, however, this will be rarely possible: 12 Much of the research on conduct disorder has failed to consider the differences between the two disorders even though there is now considerable evidence for basic differences in their causes, correlates and consequences. The frequent failure of researchers to distinguish between the two disorders often makes it difficult to know to which group, if either, the findings pertain. (p.35) W A conceptual framework that aids in the understanding of conduct disturbances can be best provided by reviewing the extensive work of Patterson (Patterson, 1976, 1982, 1984, 1986a, 1986b, 1986c; Patterson, Dishion, & Bank, 1984), at the Oregon Social Learning Center. His clinical work and observational studies on the development of "antisocial" children, spanning over the past 20 years, represents perhaps the most extensive and comprehensive research on conduct disorders. The observational research allows for one to enter the family domain and explore the "nuts and bolts" of family interactions. His research "provides one of the rare hard data looks at the family system and the operating or functional levels within such a system" (Wahler, 1982, p.82). Patterson is a proponent of a "social interactional" perspective where the stimulus-response (S-R) associations are no longer the fundamental units of analysis. Rather, it is the patterning or relationships among these S-R events that are the basic units of study. Thus, the focus is on the specific patterns of family interactions, or rather the extended patterns of action and reaction (i.e., a succession of S-R events). From this perspective, each family member’s behavior serves as both a response to an antecedent event, as well as a stimulus or antecedent event for subsequent 13 behavior. The underlying assumption of Patterson’s (1982) "coercion theory", shared by other behavioristic theories, is that the determinants of behavior (e.g., antisocial behavior) can be identified through the monitoring of the target behaviors’ objectively observable antecedents and consequences (Robinson & Jacobson, 1987). Patterson’s " coercion theory" has its root in his clinical work (e.g. , Patterson, 1975; Patterson & Reid, 1973) with the families of antisocial boys. The focus of the clinical approach was on parent training and this emphasis on parental behaviors/characteristics and responsibilities remains central to his conceptualizations. From this clinical work and subsequent research has emerged the "conviction" that the parents’ child-rearing practices (i.e., their "family management skills") play a major role in determining both prosocial and antisocial child behaviors. Patterson’s (1982, 1986a, 1986c) emphasis on the critical role of "inept family management skills" in the development of antisocial behavior demonstrates this conviction. "The parents’ failure to teach reasonable levels of compliance sets in motion a process of coercive exchange with family members. The effect of these exchanges later generalize to the school setting to produce academic failure and poor peer relations" (Patterson, 1986a, p.433). At the heart of the conceptual foundation is Patterson’s position that it is the family that becomes a training ground for the child’s antisocial behavior. There is the underlying assumption that antisocial behavior is learned and that this education takes place within the myriad of family interaction sequences. It is Patterson’s position that there are patterned exchanges of social interactions (i.e., "microsocial :18". r I... A, 1.4“ l4 exchanges") which provide the vehicle for training. The families of conduct-disturbed boys are characterized by their "coercive" interactions (i.e., they attempt to employ "control-through-pain" techniques). In other words, these families are not characterized by cooperation and other prosocial behaviors, but rather it is as if family members attempt to "bully" one another as they frequently engage in aversive behaviors. It is assumed that the parents’ failure to control and limit these negative "microsocial exchanges" provides for the basic training ground for antisocial behavior. According to Patterson (1986a): The assumption central to the general model is that failure by parents to effectively punish garden variety, coercive behaviors sets into motion interaction sequences that are the basis for training in aggression. The process set into motion involves family members in patterned exchanges of aversive behaviors; the exchanges are such that both members train each other to become increasingly aversive. This process is labeled coercion. (p.436) The model is truly interactive, as each family member simultaneously shapes the behavior of the other and at the same time is being shaped by the family interaction. For example, as the child becomes more coercive, he/she becomes more difficult to discipline, thus enhancing the effects of the inept disciplinary practices. Relatedly, Patterson (1986) pointed out that the identified deviant child can be seen as both residing in and contributing to a behaviorally disordered family system. In Patterson’s (1976) words the "child is both victim and architect of a coercive system." 15 Again, the focus is on the patterned irritable exchanges between the problem child, his mother, and siblings: The effect of these negative microsocial exchanges is to Shape the behavior of aggressor and victim simultaneously. The training in coercion or pain-control techniques is in turn thought to be a necessary prelude to training in physical fighting. It is assumed that the first or basic training phase is the result of persistent failure of the parents to use effective discipline in controlling coercive exchanges among family members. The patterned irritable exchanges increase in frequency and amplitude, which further disrupts parental efforts to discipline. (Patterson, Dishion, & Bank, 1984, p. 254) A few evaluative comments regarding Patterson’s conceptualization will aid in elaborating his position. First, the above quotation clearly exemplifies the hypothetical tie between aversive behaviors and actual aggression or assault. Patterson (1986) does suggest that "these children Show a regular progression from learning to be noncompliant to learning to be physically assaultive" (p.436). As the rates of coercion increase, the length of the coercive interactions increase and thus "...presumably, increases in the amplitude of aggression are also likely to be reinforced" (p. 436). This is the essence of Patterson’s (1982) "escalation hypothesis, which describes the process by which a dyad moves from the exchange of rather innocuous aversive events to higher amplitude aggressive behaviors which characterize child abuse and spouse abuse" (p. 13). 16 In order to evaluate Patterson’s research and conceptualizations it is essential to grasp the issues involved in the operationalizing of aggression. A general definition for the term aggression would involve one person inflicting pain upon another (Patterson, 1982). Yet, is every aversive event to be classified as aggression? Patterson’s stated goal is to establish an empirical foundation for a small subset of aggressive phenomena, namely those aversive events that occur in family interactions. "Within the family those events will be labeled as aggressive which can be shown to be both aversive and contingent" (Patterson, 1982, p.12) (i.e., the likelihood of an aversive event is predicated on a specific antecedent event). Much of Patterson’s (1982) observational research is based on the Family Interaction Coding System (FICS) which has 14 categories of aversive behaviors. This Total Aversive Behavior (TAB) score serves as a general measure of the level of coerciveness for the problem child, as well as each family member. The behaviors included are negativism, noncompliance, teasing, yelling, whining, issuing a negative command, crying, disapproval, dependency, ignoring, engaging in repetitive behavior to the extent that it becomes annoying, humiliating, destruction of property, and physical aggression. Obviously, these fourteen events do not refer to commonly defined high- intensity aggression like shooting or stabbing. Patterson’s ( 1982) position, however, is that these fourteen aversive events "...are not an unwanted concern with trivia..." (p.13) because it is hypothesized that it is in fact these ubiquitous family events that can escalate to high intensity aggression. Clearly, these fourteen behaviors reflect a wide spectrum of aversive events employed in a contingent manner by family 17 members. Patterson (1982) believes that while these behaviors differ in topography, (e. g, cry versus physical assault) they all reflect a common process and are members of a common set: Members of this set share several characteristics: 1) they are aversive; 2) they are used contingently; 3) they produce a reliable impact upon the victim; and 4) the reaction of the victim has both a short-term and a long-term effect upon the aggressor. (Patterson, 1982, p.13) Robinson and Jacobson (1987) are skeptical of the actual support for this "escalation hypothesis". They state that physical aggression is a relatively rare event and that even verbal aggression is not a high-base-rate behavior during a l-hour home observation. The scarcity of these behaviors creates problems for a functional analysis since it may be difficult to find enough behavior to evaluate. Robinson and Jacobson (1987) state that "coercion theory circumvents this problem by studying a class of behaviors termed aggressive that are not, at face value, obviously aggressive. These behaviors are called coercive, but perhaps a more neutral term would be aversive" (p.132). In stating this critique it is as if they have ignored Patterson’s stated goal to investigate a specific subset of aggressive behavior, those involved in family interactions. While Robinson and Jacobson do give credit that Patterson’s research establishes an operational basis for the study of punishing events in family interactions, nevertheless, they question whether this constitutes a basis from which to study aggression. Their final conclusion regarding the escalation hypothesis does seem warranted: While children who are reported to be aggressive engage in Ii ..- ‘Ar .,}‘ \ ..ru 18 significantly higher levels of annoying behavior (Loeber & Schmaling, 1985a; Patterson, 1982), "...the chain of events that leads to full-scale aggression has not yet been delineated and remains hypothetical" (Robinson & Jacobson, 1987,p. 133). Understanding the escalation hypothesis involves grasping the critical role of negative reinforcement. In fact, it is the principal mechanism in coercion theory. Negative reinforcement is an arrangement in which an aversive event is followed by a response which, in turn, has a neutral or positive outcome. The child’s behavior, as well as his/her parent’s, is said to be negatively reinforced when that behavior results in the termination of some kind of noxious/aversive event. When the parent " gives in" to some kind of aversive child behavior (e.g. , nagging) then the parent is negatively reinforced for acquiescence because the aversive child behavior has stopped. Similarly, when a parental request/command is met with an aversive noncompliance and then not followed through, the child is negatively reinforced since the noxious stimuli (i.e., the parental command) has been removed. While Patterson acknowledges that positive reinforcement may play a role in the development of antisocial behavior, coercion is really based on this negative reinforcement arrangement. "In distressed families, the three-step, escape-avoidant arrangements (attack-counterattack-positive outcome) may occur hundreds of times each day" (Patterson, 1986a,p. 436). It is these interactions that form the basis of the coercion hypothesis. "Negative reinforcement is not only responsible for the maintenance of many coercive events, it is also the means by which the aggressor trains a victim to comply or submit" (Patterson, 1982, p. 141). In terms of the 5‘9! .-‘ _.‘ 905511 t-“‘ l9 escalation hypothesis, Patterson (1982) speculates that there is a relationship between negative reinforcement and the escalation in the intensity of aggression: "Given a successful outcome for an increase in amplitude, negative reinforcement increases the likelihood of a high-density response with future presentations of that same stimulus" (p. 159). Patterson (1982) does present some interesting descriptive data to support his notion that in both normal and clinical samples, " . . .the stage is frequently set for the possibility of a negative reinforcement arrangement" (p. 148). In other words, the child frequently finds him/herself in the position of "counterattack" (i.e. , reacting to aversive antecedents). A computer was used to identify the antecedent events supplied by each family member to the target child’s coercive behaviors. The results indicated that given a coercive response in the child, there was "a slightly higher" likelihood of an aversive antecedent for the sample of "out of control boys" (.32), as compared to the matched non-problem sample (.25). Results from another sample of "social aggressors" again demonstrated that about one-third of the conduct disturbed child’s aversive behaviors were reactions to aversive events and not initiations. "Clearly, then, much of children’s aggression is not attack but counterattack behavior" (Patterson, p. 148). Additionally, what is the likelihood that an aversive antecedent will be presented to the child? Given that the child is engaged in a prosocial behavior, what is the likelihood that a family member will "crossover" (i.e., launch an unprovoked attack)? In Patterson’s study, the clinical sample was made up of two groups, stealers \n“ N \ 20 and social aggressors. The results indicated that each family member was significantly more likely than their normal counterparts to "crossover" and initiate an attack. Mothers were more likely than fathers or siblings to engage in crossover attacks. In the social aggressor sample, mothers responded to the child’s prosocial behavior with an aversive response 5 % of the time, as compared to only 2% for the normal mothers. Finally, while the problem children were significantly more likely to crossover towards their mothers than normal children, the problem child was less likely than their own parents to initiate a conflict. Patterson interpreted this data as supporting the position that a child in the clinical sample must learn to cope with aversive intrusions by other family members and if the coping is successful in terminating these intrusions, then "this would mean that negative reinforcement mechanisms play an important role in maintaining coercive child behavior" (Patterson, 1982, p. 150). In fact, Patterson reported that there was a significantly greater proportion of coercive behavior maintained by negative reinforcement schedules in the clinical sample (around 21%) compared to the normal sample (15%). Some skepticism about the critical role of negative reinforcement has been raised (e.g., Knutson, 1982; Robinson & Jacobson, 1987; Wahler & Dumas, 1986). First of all, while the proportion of coercive behavior maintained by negative reinforcement is significantly different between groups, Patterson does not take into account discrepant base rates of aversive behavior among aggressive and control families. Given that there are higher rates of aversive behavior among all family 21 members within the problem families, the aggressive child’s deviant behavior is more likely to be associated with the termination of aversive behavior by chance alone (Knutson, 1982). Robinson and Jacobson ( 1987) suggested that while the differences in the proportion of coercive behavior maintained by negative reinforcement are significant, they represent small absolute differences. Furthermore, on a strictly conceptual basis, Knutson (1982) pointed out that all aversive/conflictual interchanges must eventually end, and hence negative reinforcement of something will inevitably take place. Inasmuch as aversive events are a ubiquitous occurrence, then so is negative reinforcement. Robinson and Jacobson (1987, p. 139) concluded that "negative reinforcement may not be causally related to deviant behavior but may be an artifact of its definition." The fact that negative reinforcement plays such a central role in Patterson’s coercion theory might suggest that the overall conceptualization needs to be viewed with some caution. While Wahler & Dumas (1986) also acknowledged some conceptual difficulties with negative reinforcement (e.g., it is unable to account for the long-term maintenance of some coercive exchanges) they nevertheless point out that the literature broadly supports the coercion hypothesis. This conclusion, in general support of Coercion Theory, will be further substantiated by reviewing the empirical literature on family interactions. The review of the empirical literature will demonstrate that there are differences in the quality of family interactions that characterize those families with a conduct disturbed child, as compared to normal 22 families. These differences are frequently in the direction that would be predicted by Coercion Theory. W. The overall impression is quite clear; namely, there are a variety of parental characteristics and family interaction patterns that characterize families with conduct disordered children (Hetherington & Martin, 1986; Olweus, 1980; Patterson, 1982; Rutter & Garmezy, 1983; Rutter & Giller, 1983; Snyder & Patterson, 1987;). The support for these conclusions comes from an established body of research that includes observational studies, both in the laboratory and home setting, that have attempted to delineate and specify the actual maladaptive interactive processes that correlate with the global questionnaire and interview data (e.g., Patterson, 1982). It is important to point out that the most popular methodology is correlational and hence definitive statements regarding cause and effects are still lacking. Recently, however, path analytic models have been employed (e. g. , Olweus, 1982; Patterson, 1986a) providing for a step towards causal statements. Rutter and Giller (1983) suggested that this accumulation of data can be organized around six significant dimensions/factors that are involved in the mediation of family influences on the development of conduct disorders: (a) characteristics of the parents; (b) intra-farnilial discord; (c) weak relationships with parents; ((1) ineffective supervision and regulation of the child’s behavior and activities; (e) socioeconomic conditions; and (f) family size. While the present study is focused primarily on factors (a) and (d), obviously all of the six factors are interrelated. For example, it seems reasonable to 23 assume that the overall family atmosphere and quality of relationships (i.e. , factors (b) and (c) ) are reciprocally interdependent with the ways in which the parents perform their parental responsibilities. Thus, given the reciprocal nature of the influences that characterize the family (i.e., the family-systems’ effect) any categorization of the family influences involved in the development of a conduct disorder is solely for the sake of convenience. Snyder and Patterson’s (1987) survey of the literature is specifically relevant for the present study because they organized the family interaction data with an emphasis on parental behaviors/responsibilities. They organized the relevant family interaction patterns and characteristics associated with a conduct disorder around five themes: (a) discipline, (b) positive parenting, (c) monitoring, ((1) conflict and problem solving, and (e) sociodemographic characteristics. Within each of these themes a variety of cross sectional and longitudinal studies were reviewed, noting how much of the variance in the antisocial behavior had been accounted for by the variety of measures used to assess each theme. Overall, Snyder and Patterson ( 1987) concluded: The data consistently suggest that parenting practices and family interaction are associated with the development of antisocial and delinquent behavior. These findings are consistent in studies using cross sectional and longitudinal designs, and are consistently replicated using different modes of measuring the various parenting and family interactional constructs. The same variables are implicated whether delinquency [or antisocial behavior] is defined using self- 24 report measures, [observational studies] or official arrests and convictions. (p. 233) I' .10.__.r' -. . .‘. viw f - . urrrr..- Li 1 - - On FIJ' ; I n This study is concerned with the effects of parental cognitive factors as they relate to the development and maintenance of the child’s conduct disorder. The literature to be reviewed in this section will clearly demonstrate that parental discipline, and the component behaviors involved in its effective implementation, is consistently associated with the child’s conduct disturbance (Snyder & Patterson, 1987). It is very likely that parental cognitive factors play a crucial role in the parent’s disciplinary behaviors. In order to develop and advance this position, it will be essential to identify those parental/ family characteristics that differentiate families with a conduct disturbed child from normal families, emphasizing those component behaviors involved in disciplinary practices. In the next section on Parental Cognitive Variables, the issue as to whether these parental/family differences can be accounted for, in part, through an understanding of the parent’s cognitive processes will be addressed. For now, it will be helpful to provide a conceptual schema for discipline. What is discipline? And, what are the parental and family characteristics that might be relevant factors in terms of the effectiveness of the disciplinary practices? Snyder and Patterson (1987) pointed out that "discipline is a complex construct that refers to methods used by family members, [primarily parents], to discourage behavioral 25 excesses or antisocial behavior in children" (p.220). They provided three aspects involved in effective methods of discipline: (1) the accurate definition and labelling of certain behaviors as excessive or antisocial; (2) the consistent tracking of those behaviors over time and across settings; and (3) the consistent and contingent use of effective but not harsh methods to inhibit those behaviors. (p. 220) Similarly, Berkowitz (1973) suggested that the timing, consistency, and intensity of the parental response are key factors in the effectiveness of discipline. There is another class of significant variables that are likely involved in the efficacy of discipline, namely, the overall family atmosphere or emotional tone, including the quality of relationships. The interpersonal context of the discipline is a critical factor. Parental punishment is more effective in suppressing undesirable behavior when it is administered by a warm, nurturing, and loving parent, as compared to a cold, harsh, or negative parent (Berkowitz, 1973; Wahler, 1969). Snyder and Patterson (1987) referred to this factor as "positive parenting " and suggested that it may be directly involved in enhancing the "reinforcement value" of the parent. Relatedly, the frequency with which the parent responds to and values prosocial behavior may be an important factor in terms of discipline. Berkowitz (1973) pointed out that there is a likelihood of inhibiting a deviant behavior by strengthening responses that are incompatible with that behavior. Kazdin (1982) referred to the phenomenon of "response covariation" (i.e., two or more behavioral 26 responses are related). There are covariations among the responses that comprise a child’s behavioral repertoire. When responses are related, changes in one response affects the other related responses. The suggestion is that prosocial and antisocial behaviors may be part of a response covariation. Wahler and Dumas ( 1987) pointed out that "it is evident from the observational studies that the organization of a child’s prosocial behaviors with respect to his or her problem behaviors is sometimes predictable" (p. 593). Russo, Cataldo, and Cushing (1983) did report that problem children that exhibited the intended goal of an increase in compliance also exhibited an incidental reduction in their problematic behaviors. The research that differentiates the parents/families of conduct disordered children from normal families will now be reviewed. The review will highlight the following component behaviors/interactions that are suggested to be involved in effective discipline: family atmosphere, parental attitudes and emotional tone, and the contingent and non-contingent reinforcement of both prosocial and deviant behavior. (The literature on parental labeling and tracking of child behavior will be reviewed separately in the subsequent section on parental cognitive variables.) The overall family atmosphere and the parents’ attitudes towards their children do seem to be directly related to the presence of child behavior problems. While both normal and deviant families generally exhibit more positive than negative behavior towards their children, regardless of the child’s behavior (Snyder, 1977; Wahl, Johnson, Johansson & Martin, 1974), the families of antisocial children do exhibit a significantly greater amount of aversive or displeasing behaviors (Patterson, 1982; 27 Snyder, 1977). Snyder (1977) observed the behavioral sequences and interactions within both normal and conduct disordered families. The data revealed that 4.7 % of the behaviors emitted in problem families (e.g. , between father, mother, and son) were displeasing, and this was two times the rate observed in normal families. Each family member was significantly higher than the normal counterparts in exhibiting aversive behaviors (Patterson, 1982; Snyder, 1977). These results are characteristic of an overall negative and aversive family atmosphere. This characterization supports Patterson’s (1982) concept of a bilateral trait which implies that the child’s performance of coercive behavior is the outcome of two dispositions. Each member brings his or her own disposition to act irritably. Therefore, the bilateral hypothesis requires that one examine not only the child’s reactions to others, but also the reactions of family members to the child. In fact, Patterson ( 1982) did report that measures of the mother’s irritable reactions to the child account for over 40% of the variability in the child’s TAB score. Patterson suggested that "these findings support the idea that the problem child’s performance level is related in some fashion to the manner in which other persons react to him" (p.256). He concluded that aggressive children do live in families with aggressive people. This conclusion is firmly supported in the literature and is generally congruent with the position of Coercion Theory. Family interactions involving behaviorally disturbed children, especially when the parents are involved, are consistently and repeatedly characterized by their emotional negativity (Delfini, Bemal, & Rosen, 1976; Johnson, Wahl, Martin, & 28 Johansson, 1973; Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b; Olweus, 1980), by their criticalness and rejecting qualities (Forehand, King, Peed, & Yoder, 1975; Olweus, 1980; Patterson, 1986b), and by higher levels of irritability (Patterson, 1982). Many studies have emphasized the frequency/rates and quality of parental commands (Delfini et al., 1976; Forehand et al., 1975; Forehand, Well & Sturgis, 1978; Johnson & Lobitz, 1974; Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b) as indicative of the overall negative and controlling family atmosphere. Olweus (1980) employed a path analysis on two different representative samples of Swedish boys, one group age 13 and the other 16 years old, in order to examine the familial and temperamental determinants of aggression in adolescent boys. Retrospective interview data were scored for the parental variables (i.e. , negativism, pernrissiveness for aggression, and the use of power-assertive discipline methods) as well as the determination of the boy’s temperament. Aggression scores were independently obtained via peer ratings. The mother’s negativism was a composite score which characterized the principal caretaker’s basic emotional attitude towards the boy in the first five years of life. Mother’s permissiveness (an accounting of discipline style) and mother’s negativity were the most important causal factors, as the entire causal model accounted for between 21% and 33 % of the variance in the boy’s aggression. "If the mother is negative, rejecting and indifferent to the boy when he is young (and later), this is likely to result in the adolescent boy becoming relatively more aggressive and hostile toward his environment" (Olweus, 1980, p.651). There was also a substantial indirect effect of mother’s negativism and the 29 use of power assertive disciplinary techniques such as strong threats and corporal punishment on the development of higher adolescent aggression levels. Clearly, the study demonstrated that the basic emotional attitude of the mother and the lack of clear limits, or rather an inept discipline style, are directly involved in the development of a child’s conduct disturbance. Patterson (1982) pointed out that the difficult child, in conjunction with an overly permissive, highly irritable mother is a familiar theme evidenced in the literature on conduct disorders. Lobitz and Johnson (1974) have demonstrated a causal relationship between an increase in both parental commands and negative responding with an increase in child deviancy. Parents of normal children were asked to attempt to make their children look bad on certain days of home observation. The results showed that parents can raise deviancy levels in normal children, even when the effect of child compliance was removed from the deviancy score, by increasing the rate of negative responding and the rate of commands. Lobitz and Johnson (1975a) reported similar results from a follow-up study which included a sample of deviant children. A reduction in the problem child’s deviant behaviors, from a base rate, was associated not only with a decrease in parental negativeness but also with an increase in parental positiveness. Lobitz and Johnson (1975b) concluded that it appears as if deviant children "...were being subjected to a greater proportion of negative feedback and parental control than their non-deviant counterparts, regardless of whether their behavior was deviant or non-deviant" (p. 368). Lobitz and Johnson (1975b) analyzed the parent’s 30 response to child deviant behavior and child nondeviant behavior separately. The parents of problem children were significantly more negative to both deviant and nondeviant behavior than were the parents of the normal children. Thus, the parental negativity was associated not solely with deviant behavior per se, but rather with the child’s deviancy status/label. These parents also issued significantly more commands to their children. Relatedly, Delfini et a1. ( 1976) found that the parents of deviant boys produced a significantly greater number of negative commands (i.e., their commands were stated in an angry, threatening, humiliating, or nagging manner). The rate of overall commands was correlated with disruptive behavior in the deviant group but not in the normal group. It is the quality of the parental commands that appears to be a relevant variable. Forehand, Wells, and Sturgis (1978) found that fewer vague parental commands and more parental rewards issued in a free play laboratory task, together accounted for 55% of the total variance in the child’s observed compliance in the home setting. These studies demonstrate that the child’s deviant behavior and/or deviancy status is consistently related to both parental negativity/criticalness and parental control or commands. It has already been mentioned above that the mother’s irritability accounted for the largest proportion of variance in the child’s performance of antisocial behavior. It is interesting to note Patterson’s (1982) four measures of irritability: crossover, counterattack, punishment acceleration, and continuance. Crossover, mentioned above, refers to the start-up of a conflict, given that the other person is reacting in a neutral or positive fashion. Counterattack, also mentioned above, refers to the 31 second step in the process (i.e., once there is a start-up) the person has a choice to ignore or attack back. Given an initiation by a family member and a counterattack, punishment acceleration refers to the likelihood that the aversive exchange will continue. The final measure, continuance, refers to the disposition to persevere in one’s irritable reactions regardless of the response of the other family members. Family members of antisocial children were significantly higher on the measures of crossover, counterattack and continuance than their matched normal counterparts. For punishment acceleration there were nonsignificant trends for mothers, fathers, and siblings. The antisocial child, however, did Show significantly greater punishment acceleration than normal children. (This issue of responsivity to punishment will be addressed below.) In terms of the continuance measure, comparisons across three samples (e.g. , normals, stealers, and aggressors) were made for six dyadic units (e.g. , mother interacting with child, child with mother, child with father, father with child, child with siblings, and siblings with child). With the exception of the sibling interactions involving the target child, all other comparisons showed a significantly greater tendency in the clinical groups to continue negative/aversive interactions regardless of how the other members of the dyad reacted. The data just reviewed highlights the negative/aversive ambience which characterizes the families with an antisocial child. Conduct disordered children appear to come from generally dysfunctional families. The data on parental negativeness, criticalness, and family irritability does seem to offer further support for Patterson’s ( 1982) bilateral hypothesis, mentioned above, which stresses the 32 importance of the family members’ reactions to the target child. Furthermore, it is reasonable to suggest that the data can be seen as supportive of Coercion Theory, which would predict that as parents become more negative so do their children and vice versa, in a reciprocal escalation of the use of aversive events to control each other. Clearly, the correlational evidence is supportive of this position. In fact, this relationship between parental negativity (taken as a class of aversive responses) and problematic child behavior has also been demonstrated in normal families (Johnson et al., 1973; Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b) where the best overall predictor of the rate of child deviance was the parent’s overall negativism. There is research to suggest that the most effective parental response to a child’s aversive behavior is no response at all (Patterson, 1982; Sallow, 1972). Patterson (1982) reported that for both the clinical and normal samples, the child would perform a single coercive act and then stop 66% of the time, given no parental punishment. Johnson et a1. (1973) reported, however, that even with the normal sample, the more deviant child gets more attention, either positive or negative, no matter whether he is being deviant or nondeviant. Patterson (1982) reported that parents of antisocial children tend to discipline more. However, "in effect, their attempts to punish coercive child behavior made things worse" (p.130). Patterson (1982) frequently observed a pattern where the parent nags and threatens but seldom follows through so in the long run they do not "train" the child in appropriate behavior. These parents are aversive without the inhibitory effect. "Nattering" is Patterson’s term for this ineffective parental discipline. 33 Hence, while deviancy does get attention, this parental attention does not generally have the effect of suppressing the ongoing deviant behavior. It is therefore important to look at what are the differences in the child’s responsivity to parental punishment/consequences. Snyder (1977) reported that in normal families there is an overall decelerative effect, (i.e, aversive consequences to displeasing behaviors tend to decrease the rate of the behavior under baseline). For the problem families, however, this was not true. For the family unit overall, aversive consequences to displeasing behavior increased the rate of displeasing behavior over baseline. Snyder (1977) concluded: . ..that problem and nonproblem families can be differentiated on the basis of their responsivity to consequents. In nonproblem families, punishment was associated with a suppression of displeasing behavior. In problem families, punishment was associated with an acceleration rather than a suppression of displeasing behavior. (p. 534) In terms of the target child, however, parental discipline had an acceleration effect for both normal and problem boys, (i.e,, aversive consequences to deviant behavior tends to increase the rate of the deviant behavior (Snyder, 1977). Patterson (1982), however, reported that in normal families the impact of aversive consequences varied as a function of the class of aversive responses. The normal sons’ overall deviant behavior, as indicated by the TAB score, was slightly accelerated following parental punishment. Parental discipline was effective towards suppressing both social aggression (e. g., physical negative and teasing) and hostility (e. g., disapproval, 34 whine, and negativism). On the other hand, the aversive reactions by the parents of antisocial children did not effectively suppress ongoing aversive behavior in any of the specified response classes. In a direct comparison between the samples, conduct disordered boys were significantly more likely to persist in aversive behavior, given a negative consequence, than were the normal boys (i.e., the deviant children were significantly more likely to accelerate when punished (Patterson, 1982; Snyder, 1977)). Snyder ( 1977) suggested that the lack of responsivity is the result of a history of noncontingent reinforcement. It is reasonable to assume that the relative ineffectiveness of the parental discipline may be related to its random, haphazard, and noncontingent employment. Furthermore, Wahler (1969) suggested that a lack of contingent reinforcement also has an effect on family members responsiveness to social reinforcers (i.e. , family members’ prosocial behaviors). Wahler (1969) demonstrated, in a two case clinical study, that increasing the parental reinforcement value (i.e., the parents’ effectiveness) was accomplished by training the parents to be more contingent with their positive and negative reinforcement. This increase in contingent reinforcement coincided incidentally with an increase in the positive interactions between child and mother. The literature regarding the parents’ contingent and noncontingent responses consistently demonstrates that parents of deviant children are generally more noncontingent in their responses. Studies by Wahl et a1. (1974) and Snyder (1977) offered empirical support that nonproblem families do generally discriminate in their 35 responses to deviant or prosocial behavior. While there was some tendency for positive reinforcement of deviant behavior, deviant behavior received significantly more negative and neutral consequences and significantly fewer positive consequences than did nondeviant behavior. Clearly, nonproblem families appear to respond contingently. Even though Johnson et a1. (1973) provided further support that normal parents do discriminate between deviant and nondeviant behavior, they nevertheless provided interesting evidence to support the notion that family members can be characterized by a response class, or a generalized tendency to respond in a given manner regardless of the antecedent. The best support for this notion was in terms of a negative response tendency. "A tendency toward negative responding may be shown irrespective of prior child behavior" (Johnson et a1. 1973, p. 51). Families with problem children, on the other hand, do not appear to respond contingently. Parents of deviant children are significantly more likely to provide positive reinforcement for negative behavior (Patterson, 1982; Sallows, 1972/1973; Shaw, 1971/1972; Snyder, 1977) and are more likely to punish prosocial behavior (Patterson, 1982; Shaw, 1971/1972). Patterson’s measure of "crossover" is an index of punishing prosocial behavior. On the basis of sequential analysis of 10 normal families and 10 families of behavior problem children, Snyder (1977) concluded: In the problem families there were no contingencies. The probability of receiving a positive, neutral or aversive consequent was independent of the behavior displayed. Members from nonproblem families were more discriminating. Their responses fostered the development of prosocial 36 behavior and discouraged the display of deviant behavior. For a member from a nonproblem family, it paid to act in a way that was pleasing to other families members. In problem families, the payoff was nearly the same for both pleasing and displeasing behavior. (p. 533) Overall, this review of family interactions and the examination of the component behaviors involved in the parent’s disciplinary practices (i.e. , the overall aversive family atmosphere, the parents’ negative, critical, and controlling attitudes, and the greater tendency towards the noncontingent reinforcement for both prosocial and deviant behavior) would strongly suggest that the parental disciplinary practices of behaviorally disturbed children are relatively ineffective when compared to the parents of normal children. Therefore, it will come as no surprise that the effectiveness of the parent’s discipline appears to be related to the development of a conduct disturbance. In their review of the literature, Snyder and Patterson, (1987) reported that "the various measures of discipline in these studies account for between 1 to 40 percent of the variance in child antisocial behavior. Discipline is modestly but significantly related to official delinquency, aggression, stealing, and drug use" (p. 221). Relatedly, recent studies examining family-management skills, in which discipline plays a major role, also suggest that inept family management is critically involved in the development of conduct disorders (Patterson, 1986a,1986b,l986c; Patterson & Dishion, 1985; Patterson, Dishion, & Bank, 1984; Patterson & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1984). The family management skills that have been studied 37 include: (a) discipline, (b) monitoring (i.e., knowing where, when, and with whom the child is with), (c) positive reinforcement/parenting (i.e., the overall positive tone and contingent reinforcement of prosocial behavior), and ((1) problem solving. The intercorrelations of these variables were examined (Patterson, 1986c; Patterson & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1984) and the significantly positive correlations suggested that they are defining a common trait. Disruptions in one kind of parental practice appears to be accompanied by disruptions in other practices as well. The highest correlation was between discipline and positive parenting. The parents’ ineffectiveness in their implementation of discipline appears to be related to a lack of prosocial interactions with their children. This would be consistent with Wahler’s (1969) clinical study reviewed above. While Patterson and Stouthamer-Loeber (1984) reported that parental monitoring accounted for the most variance in the path analysis model, the discipline variable was also shown to make a significant contribution in the two measures of delinquent activity. The parental monitoring variable was also found to be significantly related not only to delinquency, but also to relationships with deviant peers and deficient social skills (Patterson & Dishion, 1985). In another path analysis, Patterson, Dishion, and Bank (1984) reported that "inept parental discipline" was a significant variable. The path from inept discipline to "negative microsocial exchanges" (i.e., aversive sibling interactions) was statistically significant in a bidirectional process. The effect of inept parental discipline on the antisocial criterion of physical fighting was mediated by the negative microsocial exchanges. This was 38 taken as supportive of the position that the family becomes a training ground for aggression. The weakness of the study, however, was that the construct of Discipline was defined by only a single indicator. In a recent study, Patterson’s (1986a) stated goal to use multiple methods and multiple agents to define each construct was accomplished in regards to the construct of "inept discipline". Once again, the study supported the importance of discipline in terms of the child’s conduct disturbance. The strongest path was from inept discipline to antisocial behavior, as this accounted for approximately 40% of the variance. Also, the parent’s inept discipline was bi-directionally related to the target child’s coercive exchanges with other family members. Patterson (1986a) interpreted the results that "something as ordinary as the level of the parents’ family-management skills" sets into motion an elaborate and complex process involving the child’s family relationship, and the child’s academic performance, and peer contacts. The path models clearly suggest that family management, and specifically parental discipline, originates these processes involved in the development and maintenance of a conduct disorder. In summary, parental characteristics and family interaction patterns consistently differentiate families with a conduct disturbed child from normal families. Clearly, the literature strongly suggests that these families are dysfunctional. The family interactions that are involved in the implementation of discipline appear to be significantly deficient in these problem families when compared to normals. Furthermore, the literature does suggest that disciplinary practices do play a crucial 39 role in terms of the development of a conduct disturbance. While the results are largely correlational, the path models do promote a more causal explanation. It is reasonable to propose that the parent’s inability to create an emotionally positive family atmosphere, as well as their difficulties in providing clear limits and the ineffective inhibition of the child’s deviant behavior (i.e., their inept family management skills) lie at the very heart of the child’s conduct disturbance. P ni 'v V ' les Research literature that emphasizes the importance of the parent’s cognitive functioning in terms of the child’s behavioral disturbance will be reviewed in this section. Data that suggest that parents of conduct disordered children do not accurately perceive the child’s behavior will be presented. Two related perceptual deficits will be discussed: (a) Parents of behavioral disturbed children do not accurately label their children as there are apparent discrepancies between the parent’s perceptions of the child’s behavior and the child’s actual behavior. The parental perceptions of the child’s adjustment do not appear to be based solely on the child’s actual behavior; and (b) parents of conduct disordered children exhibit some deficits in the categorization of prosocial, neutral, and deviant behavior. Bugental and Shennum (1984) stated that the child socialization literature has overlooked the intervening role of the parents’ cognitive factors in the interaction process. Recently, other investigators have also made note of the need to understand the role of parental cognitive variables in the development of the child’s conduct disturbance (Griest & Wells, 1983; Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b; Patterson, 1986b; 40 Robinson, 1985). For example, Griest and Wells (1983) stated: "Although this tOpic has received little attention in the past, it appears to us that future research is needed in the area of cognitive parent variables and their relation to parent’s perceptions of child behavior" (p. 41). Obviously, the ways by which a person views and understands the world around them affects the ways in which that person acts and interacts. Thus, it is only common sense that cognitive factors play a major role in determining human interactions. The ways that parents perceive their children, therefore, undoubtedly affects how the parent interacts with the child and, consequently, how the child behaves. Adhering to this line of logic, it is the position of the present study that parental cognitive factors play a critical role in the development and maintenance of a conduct disorder. The present study proposes that differences in the parents’ cognitive/perceptual behavior may be related, in part, to the differences in the component behaviors involved in the parents’ disciplinary actions. The parent’s negativity, criticalness, lack of contingent reinforcement, and overall inept discipline of the child’s behavior (reviewed in the previous section) may, in part, be the result of the ways in which the parent tracks, defines/labels, and processes the child’s behavior. Clearly, "parents who cannot accurately perceive different types of behavior will probably not respond to them in an appropriate manner" (I-Iolleran, Littrnan, Freund, & Schmaling, 1982, p. 548). Furthermore, it is reasonable to speculate that an inaccurate parental perceptual style can lead to an inaccurate 41 overall assessment of the child and this can result in a feedback loop; the child’s deviant behavior can be viewed as an attempt to be congruent with the parent’s negative label or impressions. It is reasonable to suggest that there is a reciprocal interactive process between the parental labeling of the child and the child’s actual behavior. Rickard, Graziano and Forehand (1984) suggest that "parents, by virtue of their unique relationship to the child, may serve not only to initiate but to perpetuate child deviant behavior, and negative child self-evaluations" (p. 184), as they caution against parental self-fulfilling prophecies. Support for the feasibility of this general position is provided by a creative and well executed study by Bugental and Shennum (1984). (This study has been referred to by Shaver (1984) as "having all the markings of a classic".) Bugental and Shennum (1984) have demonstrated the effects of parental attitudes and beliefs on ongoing parent-child interactions. They clearly demonstrated that certain parental beliefs (e. g., in this case beliefs regarding the causes of positive outcomes in parent- child interactions) acted as "selective filters or sensitizers" to specific child behaviors. Thus, the parental beliefs determined the nature and amount of the adult’s reaction to different child behaviors. Furthermore, there was evidence to support the prediction that adult attributions acted in a self-fulfilling fashion (i.e. , the communication patterns that followed from parental beliefs served to elicit the behavior patterns that maintained those beliefs). The parental attributions did not directly cause child behavior, but rather acted as central moderators in the transactional process. "In our conception, attributions act as cognitive filters-- 42 determining what stimuli will be responded to and in what fashion" (Bugental & Shennum, 1984, p.78). Clearly, "parental cognitive factors" can refer to a wide variety of behaviors including: perceptual styles, attitudes, beliefs, expectations, specific knowledge/information, and the accumulation of prior experiences. A few studies have examined the relationship between parent cognitive variables, perceptions of the child, and deviant child behavior. For example, Cahill (1978) reported that parents labeled their normal children as "difficult" when the child’s behavior did not meet the parents expectations. Interestingly, many parental expectations exhibited a lack of knowledge regarding normative child behavior. Addin'onally, Rickard, Graziano, and Forehand (1984) found that a scale measuring parental beliefs, attitudes, and expectations did differentiate parents’ of clinic-referred children from parents of normal children and there was some relationship between these cognitive measures and actual behavior in the home for the clinic sample. Overall, the parents’ beliefs about disciplinary practices and their knowledge of child development norms were the two most important cognitive variables that differentiated the groups. The results were taken as supportive of the possibility that some children are inaccurately labeled as deviant, when in fact the principle locus of the "problem " is not the child’s behavior, but the interpretation of the child’s behavior by the parents. In other words, both a lack of knowledge about developmental norms and/or unrealistic expectations may result in inaccurate parental interpretations of the child’s behavior. 43 P P r ' lin f ' B h vi The focus ofthe present study on parental cognitive factors emphasizes the parent’s overall perception of the child and the child’s actual behavior. There is now mounting evidence to suggest that parents do not always perceive their child’s behavior accurately (Griest & Wells, 1983; Rickard et al., 1984). In the studies reviewed in the previous section on conduct disorders, families with behaviorally disturbed children were compared with normal families. Studies did indicate that the deviant child’s observed behavior was significantly different from that of normal children (e.g., Delfini et al., 1976; Forehand, King, Peed, & Yoder, 1975; Griest, Forehand, Wells & McMahon, 1980; Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b; Patterson, 1982; Snyder, 1977). It would appear reasonable to conclude that these studies Show that parental perceptions are accurate since these children were either significantly more deviant and/or more noncompliant than the matched controls. Thompson and Bemal’s (1982) investigation into the factors associated with parental labeling of children referred for conduct disturbances also concluded that parental perceptions were accurate. Even though statistically significant differences did exist between the deviant and the normal children on behavioral variables, there was, nevertheless, considerable overlap in the rates of deviant behavior ( e.g., Delfini et al., 1976; Griest et al., 1980; Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b). In other words, there are a variety of studies that suggest that there is not a straightforward relationship between the parent’s perception of child deviancy and the child’s actual behavior. Delfini et a1. ( 1976) reported that " . . .approximately half of the deviant boys appeared no different from the normal ones 44 in the behaviors measured" (p.246). This rate of overlap is consistent with that found by Lobitz and Johnson (1975b) and suggests that parental perception of the child is not totally explained by the child’s actual behavior. Some children are clearly inaccurately labeled as deviant. In fact, Rickard, Forehand, Wells, Griest, and McMahon (1981) directly addressed the issue of differences between normal, clinic-referred behaviorally deviant, and clinic-referred behaviorally non-deviant children, as they confirmed the existence of a group of children who were labeled as deviant but exhibited behaviors comparable to the normal group. While the Clinic Deviant group was significantly more deviant in behavioral measures collected in the home, the Clinic Non-Deviant group was not different from the Non-clinic group on the behavioral measures. In terms of parental perceptions of maladjustment, however, the parents in both clinic groups perceived their children as significantly more maladjusted than the non-clinic parents. Thus, the parents’ perception of the clinic-nondeviant children was an inaccurate assessment of the childrens’ actual behavior. Parental perceptions appear to be potentially inaccurate as they are not always based on actual child behavior. There is now a body of research to suggest that the objective nature of the child’s behavior is not the sole determinant, or even the most important factor involved in the parent’s perception of the child. On the basis of their research with families of pre-teenage behavior problem children, Christensen, Phillips, Glasgow, and Johnson (1983) reported that "no significant relationship exists between child negative behavior and parental perception of child behavior problems, 45 even when the score for child negative behavior includes only behavior directed toward the parent and is weighted by the parent’s reaction to those behaviors" (p.162). (The data indicated that parental perceptions of child behavior were associated with marital discord and parental negative behavior towards the child, but not the behavior of the target child!) Lobtiz and Johnson (1975b) pointed out that while their data exhibited a great overlap on the child behavioral variables, there was not a lot of overlap on the parent perception variables. In fact, results from a stepwise discriminant analysis correctly classified 90% of both the clinic-referred and non-clinic-referred children based solely on the parent attitude measures. Parental perception of child adjustment appears to be a better discriminator of children labeled as behavioral disordered than the actual child behavior (Griest et al. , 1980). Overall, the conclusion is that there must be factors involved in the parental labeling process other than the target child’s actual behavior. Research reviewed in the previous section on discipline strongly suggest that parental negativity is associated with child/adolescent conduct disturbance. Yet, the correlations between the child’s actual behavior and the parental perception scores were negligible (Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b). This lack of a correlation, combined with frequently found overlap between "deviant" and normal groups on behavioral variables strongly suggest that ". ..factors in addition to child deviant behavior must be contributing to negative parent attitudes..." (Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b, p. 369). It is the goal of the present study to shed light on what might be contributing to the parental negativity. 46 There are two general factors that have attracted a lot of attention in the literature, namely, marital satisfaction (Christensen et al., 1983; Forehand, Brody, & Smith, 1986; O’Leary & Emery, 1984;) and parental psychopathology and/or personal adjustment (Griest et al., 1980; Rickard et al., 1981; Wahler, 1980; Wahler & Afton, 1980). The literature suggests that these factors may be more important in determining the parental perception of the child’s adjustment than the actual child behavior. It does not appear, however, that there is a direct link between these factors and the parental perception of the child’s behavior, but rather the relationship is interactive. For example, Griest et al. (1980) found that maternal perception of child behavior was the best discriminator between the clinic and nonclinic groups. These investigators then examined whether child behavior or maternal adjustment best predicted parental perceptions of their children. For the nonclinic group, maternal perceptions of child adjustment was best predicted by the child’s actual behavior. For the clinic group, on the other hand, maternal perceptions of child adjustment were best predicted by an interaction of child behavior and maternal adjustment. Similarly, Forehand et al. (1986) concluded that it is the combination of marital satisfaction and actual child behavior that are important determinants of parental perception of child adjustment. "When a child is disruptive (noncompliant, deviant or conflictual) and his/her mother is dissatisfied with her marriage, the child is perceived as more maladjusted than when the only problem is child disruption or an unsatisfactory marriage exist" (p.48). 47 Another possible factor that may explain the parent’s negativity and inaccurate assessment of the child’s behavior is the parent’s cognitive schema for the classification of deviancy and prosocial behavior (Patterson, 1982, 1986b). W The literature on the parental categorization of deviant, neutral, and prosocial behavior comes primarily from the Oregon Social Learning Center (Holleran, Littrnan, Freund, Schmaling, & Heeren, 1982a; Holleran, Littrnan, Freund & Schmaling, 1982b; Lorber, 1981; Patterson, 1982, 1986b; Schmaling & Patterson, 1984). Even though this research, in terms of methodology and breadth of results, is still in its formative stages, it is nevertheless of critical importance for the present study. A major component of the present study is based largely on the ideas and hypotheses (Patterson, 1982; 1986b) as well as the methodology (especially Holleran et al.. 1982b) contained in this emerging body of research. Most of the research to be reviewed in this section, however, is from unpublished reports (with the exception of Holleran et al., (1982b and Patterson, 1982 & 1986b) and the conclusions reached should be interpreted more cautiously and tentatively and taken as suggestive. Patterson (1986b) speculated that a parent’s disposition to react in a positive or negative fashion contingent on the behavior of the child is related, in part, to the parent’s schema for categorizing deviant and prosocial behavior. "It is assumed that parents differ in what it is that they classify as deviant (e. g. , some parents believe that temper tantrums are normal but that stealing is deviant). The deviancy-categorization process, in turn, determines which child behaviors will be monitored and which will 48 be punish " (p. 247). On the basis of years of clinical experience, Patterson (1982) speculated that there are specific qualities in the parental deviancy-categorization process of those parents with children characterized by an Undersocialized Aggressive Conduct Disorder (i.e., "Social Aggressors"): Rather than being distant and unattached, the parent of the Social Aggressor tends to be overly enmeshed and very irritable. It is hypothesized that when differentiating between coercive and normal acts, these parents will tend to be overly inclusive, i.e., they tend to classify many behaviors as deviant that parents of Normals would classify as borderline events. The mother of the Social Aggressors becomes an impassioned participant, scolding and threatening at the slightest provocation. As noted earlier, the irritable reactions make the situation worse. (Patterson, 1982, p. 249) The proposition is that these parents tend to view trivial instances of child misbehavior as worthy of high intensity scolding and these threats are not effective discipline techniques Since the parents frequently fail to follow through with their punishments (Patterson, 1982). Not only do the parents of conduct disordered children possibly have overly inclusive categories for classifying deviant behaviors, but it is hypothesized that these parents may have overly exclusive categories for classifying prosocial behaviors. Patterson does ask if the two classification schemata are independent. It is reasonable to suggest that they are not independent. As a logical parallel to Kazdin’s (1982) "response covariation", it is reasonable to propose that there is a "behavior 49 classification covariation" where a change in one of the classification schemata would affect the other related classification schemata. In other words, if the two categorization schemata are related, then an emphasis on deviancy would inherently result in a de-emphasis on prosocial behavior, or vice versa. Furthermore, if this deviancy-categorization hypothesis is valid, then parents with this overinclusive schema would be experiencing and responding to a greater amount of child deviant behavior, while experiencing less positive behavior. Clearly, this might account, in part, for the parent’s negativity, criticalness, and tendency to start a conflict when the child is behaving in a neutral or prosocial fashion (i.e. , Patterson’s "crossover"). Finally, an overly inclusive category for labeling behavior as deviant in conjunction with an overly exclusive category for defining praiseworthy behaviors, might be a critical factor in accounting for the inaccurate parental perceptions of the child’s overall behavior that was discussed above, most striking in Rickard’s et al. (1981) Clinic non-deviant group. Even though the data are limited, there is some support for the deviancy- categorization hypothesis. Lorber (1981) had the mothers of conduct disordered children and the mothers of normal children view videotapes of parent-child interactions. The mothers were asked to push different buttons to identify events as prosocial or antisocial. While viewing the identical segments of videotape interactions, mothers of aggressive children indicated that the stimulus child they were viewing emitted a significantly greater number of antisocial behaviors than did mothers of the normal children. This result would be in keeping with the hypothesis 50 that these mothers are overly inclusive in the categorization of deviant behavior and thus see more deviancy. Patterson (1986b) reported that these mothers of antisocial children classified many behaviors as deviant that were not classified as deviant by trained observers. The mothers from the two samples, however, did not differ in terms of the accuracy (determined a priori by trained behavioral observers) of detecting aversive or prosocial child behavior. Schmaling and Patterson (1984) employed a similar tracking paradigm but did not find differences in labeling processes between mothers of problem and nonproblem children. It is important to note, however, that these mothers were exposed to an initial videotape reflecting either positive or negative parent-chfld interactions. These investigators suggested that since the mothers were selectively reactive to child behavior dependent upon the type of scene they viewed first, this may account for the lack of differences in the labeling processes. Due to the initial exposure the mothers were predisposed to respond to the ambiguous child behaviors in accordance to the prevailing valence of the initial scene. Rather than using a videotape apparatus, Holleran et al. (1982b) employed a paper—and-pencil measure of parental labeling and tracking. They presented the parents with a script containing 10 short vignettes made up of a determined amount of prosocial, deviant, and neutral child behavior. The parents were asked to differentially underline events they categorized as prosocial or antisocial child behaviors. This did prove to be a useful paper-and-pencil method for evaluating the parents’ tracking behavior. These investigators, using a "signal detection format", 51 found that the parents of normal children were significantly more sensitive to prosocial behavior than were the parents of problem children. (Sensitivity was really agreement with the classification of behavior segments made by the experimenter.) This would support the contention that parents of antisocial children are overly exclusive in their categorization of prosocial behaviors. A second significant finding was found on the variable for identification of negative behaviors as positive, as parents of normal children were more prone to this error. Finally, in a pre- publication draft of the research (Holleran et al., 1982a) the investigators reported that they asked the parents of the two samples to rate statements about deviant child behavior on their aversiveness. The parents of the antisocial children rated the various behaviors as significantly more aversive than did the normal parents. Finally, the studies just reviewed did attempt to investigate whether the parents’ tracking and categorization behaviors measured in the laboratory were related to the parents’ observed behaviors in the home (Holleran et al. , 1982b; Lorber, 1981; Schmaling & Patterson, 1984). There were a variety of significant correlations between the various scores from the laboratory tasks and observational data of the mothers’ interactions with their children at home. For example, Schmaling and Patterson (1984) reported tendencies to label neutral behavior as positive on the analog videotape task were significantly correlated with the proportion of observed maternal behavior that was positive. Also, Holleran et al. (1982b) reported that lower sensitivity to negative behaviors on the tracking task was associated with higher rates of positive behaviors in the home. They suggested that the normal parents’ 52 " insensitivity" to negative behavior may, in some way, be responsible for the positive social atmosphere in the home. (It was suggested previously that sometimes the most appropriate response to negative child behavior is no response!) Overall, the results do hold promise. For present purposes, the paper—and- pencil tracking instrument appears sensitive to detect differences in parental perceptual styles and the various correlational findings with the home observation data does offer some support for the external validity and generalizability of this laboratory perception assessment paradigm. The fact that the parents of conduct disordered children rate deviant behavior as significantly more aversive is also an interesting finding. It does appear that the deviancy-categorization hypothesis is supported and seems worthy of further investigation. Additional data, especially from another research setting, would be valuable. Also, the specific sub-type of conduct disorder that makes up the sample in Holleran et al. (1982b) is not clearly specified. A worthwhile test of the overinclusivelunderinclusive categorization hypothesis would be to specifically examine the hypothesized processes in the parents of children with an Undersocialized Aggressive Conduct Disorder, focusing on the number of neutral behavioral segments that are judged to be positive or negative, as well as the deviant and prosocial behaviors that are inaccurately classified. Finally, it would be interesting to examine whether parents differ not only in terms of their ratings of aversiveness, but also in their ratings of the desirability of specified child behaviors. 53 W The present investigation proposes that the parents’ present parenting style, specifically the parental cognitive factors involved in their deviancy-categorization schemata/processes, will be associated with the parents’ recollections regarding their own childhood. The rationale for this position will be developed in this final section. It will be helpful to review what has been established in the previous sections. Clearly, there are a variety of parental qualities that are associated with the child’s conduct disturbance. Coercion theory helps to provide a conceptual basis for grasping how the parent-child interactions foster the development of antisocial behavior. The parent’s frequent and inept attempts at family management, including discipline, in fact "trains" the child along the developmental path of disturbed conduct. The child’s increasing behavioral disturbance only exacerbates the parent’s ineffectiveness. Paradoxically, the parental attempts to eliminate certain behaviors only serves to fortify them. These parents have been characterized as "nattering" or nagging, negativistic, critical, and controlling. It is suggested that these parents do not always view the child and his/her behavior accurately and other factors beyond the child’s actual performance are involved in the perceptual deficits. Clearly, the parental characterization and labeling of the child must strongly influence the child and the overall quality of the parent-cth interactions. The present study will attempt to provide further support for the deviancy categorization hypothesis, namely, that the parents of children with an Undersocialized Conduct disturbance are overly inclusive 54 in their classification schema of child deviancy and relatedly, underinclusive, (or rather overly exclusive), in their classification schema of desirable/prosocial behaviors. On the assumption that this deviancy-categorization process is in fact taking place, it is reasonable to suggest that the inaccurate classification of the child’s behavior sets into motion an elaborate process. Patterson (1986b) reported that parent’s overinclusiveness for deviant behaviors correlated with the parent’s disposition to scold and nag during home observations. The parents perceive the child negatively and there is consistent evidence supporting an association between parental negativity and child deviancy, even within normal samples. Constant inaccurate tracking and labeling of the child’s behavior can easily lead to an inappropriate, chaotic, and ineffective management of the child’s behavior. It should come as no surprise that the child appears non-responsive to parental control, and in fact parental attempts to discipline can accelerate the frequency of the child’s deviant behaviors. As these parent-child interactions increase in frequency and aversiveness/intensity, the parental perception and overall negativity towards the child appears justified and it seems likely that the child’s behavior and self-concept will inevitably be altered/modified (Patterson, 1986b). The fact that these children do exhibit a greater level of deviancy is possibly the outcome of a positive feedback 100p, or the child’s attempt to live up to the perceived parental "expectations" (i.e., to act in accordance with how their parents perceive them). 55 It should be clear that parental cognitive factors play a critical role in determining the parent-child interactions and these parent-child interactions appear to be consistently associated with the child’s conduct disturbance. Also, these parental cognitive variables play an important role in the parents’ deficient family management skills. In this final section, the hypotheses regarding the determinants of the parental behaviors will be developed. Why is it that these parents lack the necessary skills involved in effective parenting? Specifically: Why is it that these parents are overinclusive in their definition of deviancy and underinclusive in their definition of prosocial child behaviors? Even though Patterson suggested that inept family management is the result of faulty training, he is of the opinion that the parents’ negative cognitive style and their deviancy-categorization schema are very likely the result of living with a deviant child and being constantly exposed to deviant behaviors (Patterson, 1986b). The position of the present study, however, is that the parent arrives at the parenting situation with a predisposition to respond in a negative manner. The parents’ cognitive processes can be characterized by an anticipatory response set geared towards responding in a critical or negative fashion and relatedly, there is a reluctance or hesitancy to respond in a praiseworthy or complimentary mode. Therefore, while Patterson would suggest that this response set originates through the actual interaction history with the child, the present study is suggesting that the propensity to respond in a generalized negative manner is acquired prior to the initiation of parent-cth interactions. 56 It is the position of the present study that the parents’ response proclivity is the product of their representational model regarding parent-cth interactions and further, that this mental model is based largely on the parents’ own childhood experiences. Belsky (1984) has recommended that the parent’s ontogenic origins and psychological resources may be an important influence in determining the quality of the parental functioning. He reviewed literature that supports the contention: .. .that developmental history shapes personality and psychological well-being, which in turn influences parental functioning. Indeed, a hypothesis that I advance is that, in general, supportive developmental experiences give rise to a mature healthy personality, that is then capable of providing sensitive parental care which fosters optimal child development. (Belsky, 1984, p. 86) The present study proposes that parents of antisocial children had childhood experiences that were lacking, to varying degrees, in these "supportive developmental experiences", and these experiences are reflected in the functional organization of their mental model regarding parent-child interactions. Thus, it is hypothesized that the parents of antisocial children have an affective-cognitive model regarding parent- child relationships that is organized in a defensive or negative orientation, (i.e., it is oriented towards limiting and/or distorting the access or availability of specific classes of information). Underlying Belsky’s (1984) position is a widely accepted assumption, regardless of theoretical orientation, that one’s childhood experiences can affect later adult functioning, including parental functioning. For example, it is a generally held puma: lacing mg 1936:. SUCK“: general; P133213: 313mm migrat- 57 position that parents are ineffective in family management skills because they are lacking in effective role models and simply were not exposed to beneficial child- rearing practices (Patterson, l986a, 1986b; Robinson, 1985; Wahler & Dumas, 1986). It is now clearly established that there is an inter-generational continuity to coercive, antisocial behaviors as these "behavior patterns are passed on from one generation to the next at a rate well beyond chance" (Wahler & Dumas, 1986, p. 50). Patterson (1986b) interpreted these data as supporting the idea that inept family management is the result of poor training in family skills as ”...faulty parenting practices are handed down from one generation to another" (p. 440). "While the modeling principles have intuitive appeal, the processes through which a child comes to imitate a particular model remain poorly understood" (W ahler & Dumas, 1986, p. 62). Adults, and to a limited extent children, have the cognitive potential and capacity to remember, to reflect upon past experiences, to compare and integrate past and current events and ultimately, to learn from these experiences. The fact that a parent was subjected to coercive parenting as a child does not inherently lead to the repetition of this parenting style with his/her own children. Rather, exposure to a coercive model could very easily result in the parent’s determination to acquire additional information and parenting skills. While childhood experiences can be determinants of adult functioning, there can be a broad spectrum of effects ranging from the blind repetition of relationship patterns to the conscious conviction not to model or repeat the parents’ behavior. 1m 58 The present study is concerned with the ways in which parents have mentally represented and organized their own childhood experiences and the resultant affective- cognitive model regarding parent-child relationships. Bugental and Shennum (1984) pointed out that adults come to the parenting situation with a history of social experiences with children and, at the very least, they have been exposed to the model of their own parents. They suggested that these experiences are encoded as causal beliefs about the forces operating in such relationships. The point is that the adult has a pre-established organized and integrated conceptual system of these experiences (i.e. , of parent-child interactions). A few comments regarding the properties of this parent-child mental model are now in order. First, childhood experiences exist only to the extent that they are given meaning. In other words, the experiences of a child are likely to be important only in relation to the way in which the child conceptualizes his/her experiences (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Hinde, 1980). The child’s experiences are internally represented within a coherent system, referred to here as a mental (or affective-cognitive) model of parent-child relationships. It is within this organization or conceptual schema that the experiences attain their meaning. Secondly, within this representational system, the parent is not encoded solely as a role-model-to-be-imitated, but rather it is the experiences or the relationship between parent and child that are mentally represented (Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986); and this would include both affective and cognitive components. Finally, the mental model is dynamic and not static. The child’s experiences are organized schematically not categorically (i.e. , they are not encoded 59 as merely static "pictures" but rather experienced events of actions and reactions). It is in this way that the model is not perceived as a static entity. Rather, the parent- child mental model is most helpfully conceived as structured processes serving to obtain or limit access to relevant information (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). It is the goal of this study to explore how this mental or representational model is organized. Is it a model that exhibits a degree of integration and understanding regarding the parents’ own childhood experiences? Or, rather, is it a mental model that is defensive in its structure? In other words, is the model prone to distort, restrict, and/or inhibit certain classes of information? Furthermore, another goal of this study is to examine the relationship between the organization of this mental model and the parents’ current behavior, specifically their perceptual style. The present study hypothesizes that there will be a relationship between the degree of defensiveness of the parents’ recollections regarding their own childhood and their current deviancy-categorization processes. It is proposed that parents with a defensive orientation towards their own childhood will be more prone to exhibit a negative perceptual style towards their own children (i.e., they will exhibit an overly inclusive definition of deviancy and an overly exclusive definition of prosocial behavior). It is incumbent upon this presentation to address one final issue, namely: How is it that the parents’ defensive orientation towards their own childhood would be related to their current deviancy-categorization behavior? What is the link between the parents’ defensiveness regarding their own childhood experiences and the apparent negative perceptual style directed towards their own child? Brena “Ma-9 lI’LEl ‘& V» g... . ‘3‘ h 60 W. In order to approach this critical issue it will be essential to direct attention to the current status of attachment theory (Bretherton & Waters, 1985) as recent advances, both conceptually and methodologically, are extremely apropos for the present study. For example, recent reconceptualizations have focused on the representational level of attachment relationships (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). Thus, there is an emphasis on the role of cognitive/mental structures in parent-child interactions. Additionally, there is an emerging body of research to suggest that retrospective accounts of the mother’s childhood are associated with the quality of current parental behavior (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Grossman, Fremmer- Bombik, Rudolph, & Grossman, 1988; Main et a1. 1985 ; Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Morris, 1981; Ricks, 1985). It is the goal of this section to clearly articulate the conceptual logic underlying these results. This will be accomplished mostly by detailing Mary Main’s research (Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Main, et al., 1985). While the present study in not focused on attachment theory per se, recent drinking in this area does provide a conceptual foundation for the present investigation, as well as corresponding methodological support. Thus, it will be helpful to briefly review attachment theory. Attachment theory, as presented by John Bowlby (1973, 1980, 1982) is concerned with a goal-corrected motivational- behavioral control system involved in the infants’ proximity and security-seeking interactions with the mother or attachment figure. According the Bretherton (1985), the term "attachment system": 61 ...refers to a psychological organization hypothesized to exist within a person. This system is so constituted that feelings of security and actual conditions of safety are highly correlated, although the correlation is by no means perfect. Seen from an outside observer’s viewpoint the system’s set-goal is to regulate behaviors designed to maintain or obtain proximity to and contact with a discriminated person or persons referred to as the attachment figure(s). From the psychological vantage point of the attached person, however, the system’s set-goal is felt security. (p. 6) The assessment of the infant’s attachment system is conducted in the Strange Situation (Ainsworth, 1972) a laboratory paradigm of separation and reunion episodes between mother and infant. On the basis Of the observed infant behavior towards the mother during reunion, the infant’s attachment behavior can be classified as: (a) Securely attached: these infants approach the mother and seek physical contact with her if they had been distressed by the separation, or if they had not become distressed, they still greeted the mother and seek interaction and/or contact; (b) Non- attached or avoidant: these infants avoid the mother on her retum, or (c) Insecure attached: these infants exhibit angry resistent behavior mixed with seeking proximity and contact. The assessment of attachment usually takes place when the infant is between 12-18 months old. Bretherton (1985) pointed out that the validity of the Strange Situation as an assessment of the infant’s quality of attachment is derived primarily " . . .from the systematic and extensive correlations of infant behavior in this situation with maternal 62 and infant behavior in the home throughout the first year of life" (p.15). In fact, even though the Strange Situation does not directly assess the parent’s behavior, Morris (1981) pointed out that the correlations with maternal behavior are so consistent that researchers frequently equate infant classification with specific characteristics of the parent’s caretaking behaviors, implying that there are parallel individual differences in the parents. It appears that "the main variable that discriminates mothers of securely attached babies from mothers of anxiously attached babies is sensitive responsiveness to the infant’s signals" (Morris, 1981 , p.312). Mothers of securely attached infants have been independently judged to be high on maternal warmth, affectionate behaviors, and maternal sensitivity (Bretherton, 1985; Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986). On the other hand, insecurely attached and/or avoidant infants have mothers that were independently judged as rejecting and/or inconsistent to the infant’s bids for physical contact (Bretherton, 1985; Main & Goldwyn, 1984). This association between the infant’s avoidance and the mother’s rejection is of critical importance in terms of the present study. While reviewing the empirical literature on family/parental characteristics of families with a conduct disturbed child, it was consistently shown that the parent’s rejecting and negative attitude towards the child was associated with the child’s deviant behavior. It is beyond the scope of this paper to explore the implication that perhaps antisocial children were, in fact, rejected infants in regards to their attachment behavior, suggesting a developmental continuum with similar underlying processes. It is interesting to note, however, that infants avoiding their mothers at 12-18 months demonstrated a similar pattern of behavior at any i. m 3:; 63 six years-old (Main et al., 1985). Furthermore, in a follow-up study, Sroufe (1983) reported a number of aversive behavior patterns in 4-6 years—old children associated with early avoidance of the mother. Maternal rejection appears to be related to the development of unpredictable bouts of hostility, avoidance, and a lack of empathy for the distress of others (Main & Goldwyn, 1984). It is as if rejection (by the mother) fosters rejection (by the infant/toddler). Is this the same as aversiveness fostering aversiveness as proposed within coercion theory? While Patterson might account for the infant’s avoidance and the mother’s negativity, both examples of aversive behaviors, via a negative reinforcement paradigm, attachment theory offers a disparate point of view. Within attachment theory it is suggested that the infant’s avoidance of the mother is a self-protective and therefore adaptive response. It is presumed that the infant initiates physical contact/proximity (i.e., attachment behavior) when under stress and/or perceived threat, and this is alleviated by the attachment figures responsiveness. When the mother is not responsive, however, the infant’s attachment system is continually activated without termination. Bowlby proposed that continual exposure to a rejecting mother may lead, for self-protective or defensive purposes, to a deactivation of the attachment behavioral system. According to Morris (1981), the repeated rebuffs by the rejecting mothers lead to automatic withdrawal and avoidance behavior in situations that usually arouse attachment behavior in secure infants. Morris explained the automatic nature of the avoidance as "...skipping over the phase of intensified 64 proximity and contact-seeking that the infant " knows " would lead to rejection" (p. 313). Main et al. (1985) have proposed a reconceptualization of attachment processes, suggesting a shift from the behavioral level, as exemplified in the Strange Situation, to an emphasis on the representational level. It is this new focus on the representational processes and the accompanying research that is of direct relevance for the present study. This reconceptualization promotes research strategies regarding adult attachment with a focus on representation and language. Main et al. (1985) suggested that differences in attachment relationships relate to individual differences in mental representation (i.e. , the individual’s " internal working model" of attachment). (This parallels the parent’s mental model regarding parent-child relationships.) They defined the internal working model of attachment as : ...a set of conscious and/or unconscious rules for the organization of information relevant to attachment and for obtaining or limiting access to that information, that is, to information regarding attachment-related experiences, feelings and ideations. ...[these are] models that direct not only feelings and behavior but also attention, memory, and cognition, insofar as these relate directly or indirectly to attachment. Individual differences in these internal working models will therefore be related not only to individual differences in patterns of nonverbal behavior but also to patterns of language and structures of mind. (p. 67) 65 The infant’s avoidance of the mother takes on added meaning from this perspective or reconceptualization. The infant’s avoidance is seen as an organized shift of attention away from all information relevant to attachment. According to Main and Goldwyn (1984): In looking away from and ignoring the attachment figure in stress situations, the rejected infant may be seen as acting to exclude "attachment-relevant- information" from firrther processing. Presumably the infant is avoiding the pain which has been associated with repeated experiences of the rejection of her/his attachment. (p. 210) It is reasonable to speculate that the infant’s approach-avoidance conflict (i.e. , seeking proximity and avoiding nonresponsiveness or rejection) can be quite disorganizing and unsettling. Hence, the shift of attention away from the attachment figure, including potentially disturbing affects, may help in the maintenance of a self- organization or cohesiveness (Main & Weston, 1982). It is interesting to note that this emphasis on the affective-cognitive organization of attachment behavior provides for an accounting of intergenerational continuity in attachment. It is hypothesized that the internal working model of the rejected child will be organized in such a manner as to shift attention away from attachment experiences and the feelings involved in those experiences. When the rejected child is a parent, however, this organization of their internal working model would result in a similar nonresponsiveness towards their own offspring, since attachment relevant information will be excluded. It is in this way that there can be 66 intergenerational continuity. The parents have developed information-processin g or representational patterns which result in the exclusion and distortion of information, and the subsequent nonresponsiveness to the child’s attachment seeking behavior. The child then institutes Similar avoidant and self-protective affective-cognitive representational processes and the pattern of rejection continues to the next generation. The research to be reviewed below is generally supportive of this speculation regarding the intergenerational continuity in attachment. There is some empirical support for this emphasis on representational processes. Main et al. (1985) have examined the attachment behavior and the "internal working models" of attachment in a group of six year-olds and their parents. This group had been previously assessed in the Strange Situation when the same children were infants. The reunion behaviors of the six year-olds (i.e. , the security of their attachment) was significantly correlated with their security of attachment assessed during infancy. Furthermore, a variety of measures designed to assess the six-year olds’ attachment on a representational level were significantly associated with the child’s attachment classification made during infancy. For example, the child’s secure versus insecure responses to a family photograph were compared to an independently derived classification of the attachment behavior during infancy. Main et al. (1985) pointed out that "since the photograph could not respond to or control the child’s response, the child’s response to the photograph could not simply be an example of an interaction pattern being maintained over several years between child and parent" 67 (p.78). Thus, it is not actual behavioral interactions as in the Strange Situation, but rather some mental representation or mental model that must account for differences in the child’s responses to pictures of their families. Overall, the results were consistently supportive of the reconceptualization emphasizing the representational processes and the value of the " internal working model" construct. W5. Research is now emerging to support the position that a mother’s retrospective account of her own childhood is significantly related to her current parental behaviors (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Grossman et al., 1988; Main et al., 1985; Main&Goldwyn, 1984; Morris, 1981; Ricks, 1985). The recollections of childhood attachments, as assessed by either interviews (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Grossman et al., 1988; Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Morris 1981) or a questionnaire (Ricks, 1985) appear to have a significant relationship to the mother’s ability to serve as a secure base for her child. "The evidence reviewed compellingly indicates that the quality of a mother’s caregiving behavior is strongly related to her memories of childhood relationships" (Ricks, 1985, p.226). It is not only the specific content of the childhood memories but also the organizational properties of these memories that are related to current caregiving behaviors (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Grossman et al., 1988; Main et al., 1985; Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Ricks, 1985). The present study will examine the relationship between parental recollections of their childhood and their current perceptual style of child behavior (i.e., their deviancy-categorization processes). The recent studies mentioned above begin to offer support for the position that parental childhood memories are related to the 68 parents’ current behaviors. The specific link between these memories and current behavior is provided conceptually via the construct of "internal working model". The parental recollections are taken as representative of their mental model regarding attachment behavior. It is hypothesized that adults who were subjected to parental rejection during childhood will have developed representational processes organized in a manner which shifts attention away from receiving information that would activate these emotionally painful experiences. This limiting of the access/availability of specific classes of information would then account for the parents’ apparent lack of responsiveness to their own children. The parental nonresponsiveness on a behavioral level corresponds with the limiting of access to specific information on the representational level. Specific support for this position was initially provided by Mary Main’s research on adult recollections of childhood experiences (Main et al., 1985; Main & Goldwyn, 1984) and has subsequently been further supported in the literature (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Grossman et al., 1988). The design of the present study is largely based, both conceptually and methodologically, on Main’s research. Therefore, it will be essential to review this research in detail. In order to elicit the parents’ childhood recollections and assess their internal working model of attachment, the Berkeley Adult Attachment Interview protocol was developed (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1984). This structured interview was designed "to probe alternately for descriptions of relationships, specific supportive memories, contradictory memories, assessments of relationships in childhood and current w 11 Oil, 31131" 111116 it) . .‘Ir \1 ‘ I ‘3: r7. M4 a: l‘ u “it‘d 69 assessments of the same experiences and relationships" (Main & Goldwyn, 1984, p. 211). (A questionnaire format of this interview is used in the present study.) The interview protocols were initially scored for the "apparent rejection" of the mother in her own childhood. "The rating was based not only upon the parent’s direct response to questions regarding rejection, but also upon the way in which these responses were integrated into the rest of what the parent said or remembered about her experiences (Main & Goldwyn, 1984, p. 212). Additionally, the protocols were also scored for quality of the recollections and/or the degree of distortions or defensiveness. These measures were: (a) Idealization of mother: the extent to which the subject seemed to cover up or idealize an apparently neglecting, interfering, or rejecting mother; (b) Anger with mother now: this rating was for conscious, direct feelings or expressions of anger towards the mother; (c) Inability to recall childhood: this was simply the frequency with which the subject insisted that she could not remember her childhood; and (d) Coherency of the transcript: this rated the mother’s ability to talk about her past and evaluate its influence coherently. Incoherent protocols exhibited unrecognized contradictions and/or the subject frequently lost track of the topic, with interruptions in the flow of the interview conversation. The results indicated that "apparent rejection" in childhood was strongly associated with a defensive orientation toward childhood memories. Apparent rejection was positively correlated with an inability to recall, an idealization of the parent (similar to Ricks, 1985) and negatively correlated with the coherence of the prom; R1321} 70 protocol (i.e. , the less coherent the protocol the greater the likelihood of apparent rejection). The salient cognitive properties of the childhood recollections of mothers that were apparently rejected can be characterized by the distortion of information (i.e. , idealization) the disorganizing and splintering of information (i.e. , incoherent transcript) and the exclusion of information (i.e. , inability to recall). As predicted, the mothers who were apparently rejected during infancy/childhood did in fact have a mental model that was defensively organized to block access or shift attention away from attachment related information. Furthermore, the mother’s "apparent rejection" by her mother was strongly associated with her own infant’s avoidance, as assessed five years earlier (Main & Goldwyn, 1984). On the basis of the consistently strong correlations reported in the literature between the infant’s avoidance of the mother in the Strange Situation and the mother’s independently observed rejection of the infant (e.g., r > .80), Main and Goldwyn inferred that there is a strong tendency for mothers who have been rejected by their mothers to reject their own children. Additionally, Main et al. (1985) reported that the interviews were rated for the parents’ security with respect to experiences, ideas, and feelings surrounding attachment. There was a strong correlation between the infant’s security of attachment, assessed five years prior, and the implicit security in the mother’s internal working model of attachment. According to Main and Goldwyn (1984), the overall results indicated: If m .9 .-A 77!"- .. _ V b," 71 ...a systematic relationship between a woman’s (often indeliberate or unconscious) representation of her own mother as rejecting during the Berkeley Adult Attachment Interview and her evident rejection of her own infant. We find that a mother’s rejection of her own infant is related to cognitive processes which seem to distort and attempt to negate her own early experiences of maternal rejection" (p. 205). In other words, it is reasonable to interpret the data as suggesting that the mother’s apparent failure to integrate her own childhood experiences, as demonstrated in her defensively organized internal working model, is a critical determinant of parental rejection and the corresponding infant’s avoidance. For example, the mother’s inability to recall her childhood and her idealization of her parents was significantly related to her own infant’s likelihood to avoid her. On the other hand, there was a significant negative relationship between the coherence of the mother’s recollections and the infant’s observed avoidance. Further, the mother’s ability to directly experience anger and resentment, rather than a defensive idealization, was also significantly associated with a more securely attached infant. No matter what the mother’s childhood experiences, if she could speak freely and coherently of her feelings and experiences regarding her own childhood, then her infant was more likely to be securely attached and not avoidant. Grossman et al. (1988) reported strikingly similar findings. It is therefore reasonable to propose that the mother’s ability to integrate and differentiate or separate herself as an adult from her experiences as a child is a 33th flpene Mia 72 critically important characteristic in determining the quality of current parenting behaviors. To the extent that this integration has taken place, and the parents have "come to terms" with their childhood experiences, then there would be less of a self- protective need to distort and block potentially pain-evoking or unsettling attachment relevant information. In fact, Main and Goldwyn (1985) did describe one mother whose recollections of her unhappy childhood were "exceptionally coherent" . This mother had not idealized her rejecting parents, nor had she blocked from her awareness childhood experiences. Rather, she had come to terms with the experiences and had genuinely understood and forgiven her parents. This mother had the least avoidant infant in the sample. Clearly, the data support the position that it is not the overall childhood experience but rather the degree to which it has been understood and integrated that is related to the parent’s caregiving behavior. This is of pivotal importance for the present study. It is not solely the quality of the childhood relationships per se, but rather the ways in which these relationships are cognitively represented and organized that are related to current parental behaviors. To summarize, where the parent’s own experiences and feelings are not integrated, restrictions of varying types are placed on attention and the flow of information with respect to attachment. These restrictions appear in speech in the form of incoherences and in behavior as insensitivities. (Main et al., 1985, p. 100) 1':th rug. 3,9" 3111 Milan: Italic,- &13110 E31113 k . -fiflqh {‘“sd‘l 73 Thus, there is support for the position that the defensive orientation of the parent’s internal working model regarding attachment does limit access to attachment relevant information, thereby affecting parental responsiveness to the infant and the overall quality of the parent-child interactions. Crowell and Feldman (1988) offered support for this postion in an older sample of toddlers, ages 2-5 years-old. They reported an association between the mother’s internal models of relationships and the quality of the mother-child interactions observed during a semistructured laboratory play and problem-solving session. Sum. In summation, the current conceptualizations and research in attachment theory offer support for the basic tenets of the present study. Parental recollections regarding childhood are related to the parents’ current caregiving behaviors. The conceptual bridge between these parental memories and current parental behaviors is the construct of an internal mental model or structured/organized affective-cognitive processes that guide and determine, to a large extent, the quality of the parent-child interactions. The organizational properties of this parent-child mental structure appear related to the degree to which the childhood experiences were conflictual and emotionally problematic. Thus, this mental structure can be organized, for apparently self-protective reasons, in such a manner as to actively shift attention away from certain classes of information. This can result in the limiting of the flow and access of information or rather in a disruption in the organization of the information being processed. On a behavioral level, this can be evidenced as rejection and/or nonresponsiveness. rarer 2th 1m rarer: rat mater ltd :n 74 The present study is proposing that the negativity that characterizes the parents of antisocial children is a parallel of the rejection and nonresponsiveness that characterizes the mothers of avoidant infants. The present study is examining the parents’ negative perceptual style (i.e. , as reflected in their hypothesized deviancy- categorization schema) as one aspect of this overall negativity. It is hypothesized that the parental negativity is the result of a defensively organized mental model regarding parent-child relationships. It is assumed that the affective-cognitive processes are organized to shift attention away from information regarding parent-child relationships, in order to avoid potentially pain-evoking information. The negativity and criticalness of parents with a conduct disturbed child, as consistently observed in the empirical literature, is assumed to coincide on an affective-cognitive level with the exclusion and distortion in the processing of information relevant to parent-child relationships. Specifically, it is hypothesized that the parent’s deviancy-categorization schema is based on an attentional selectivity. An overly exclusive classification schema of prosocial behaviors is interpreted as a selective inattentiveness, or a Shifting of attention away from this class of information. This results in the parent’s nonresponsiveness, rejection, and even irritability or negativity (e.g., Patterson’s "crossover") to the child’s prosocial behaviors. In other words, the lack of responsiveness of the mother to the infant’s proximity and security seeking is assumed to be a parallel to the parent’s reluctance to define and thus respond positively to the child’s prosocial behaviors. Prosocial behaviors are affiliative in nature and 75 frequently promote praise, affection, and emotional closeness. It is in this way that prosocial behaviors can be seen as relationship oriented, or as "attachment relevant information". And, the parents’ overly exclusive schema for the classification of prosocial behaviors can be interpreted as a Shifting of attention away from this information. It is hypothesized that the parent’s defensively organized affective- cognitive model regarding parent-cth relationships will be related to the parent’s reluctance or hesitancy to define child behavior as praiseworthy or prosocial. On the assumption that the parental classification schemata regarding deviant and prosocial child behaviors are not independent, then the parents’ overly exclusive categories for classifying prosocial behaviors will be related to an overly inclusive category for defining deviant behaviors. The parent’s hypothesized shift of attention away from attachment related information and the subsequent overly exclusive classification schema for prosocial behaviors, results in an emphasis on classifying the child’s behavior as deviant. Hence, the selective inattention or nonresponsiveness to prosocial behaviors, coexist with a selective attentiveness to deviant behaviors. It is hypothesized that the parents’ defensively organized mental model regarding parent- child relationships will be related to their willingness/eagemess to define child behavior as deviant. Macs There are three groups of hypotheses. The first group, hypotheses 1-7, is about the mothers’ perception of child behavior. The second group, hypotheses 8-10, is focussed on the mothers’ recollections of their own childhood experiences. The 76 third group, consisting of hypothesis 11, will examine if there is a relationship between the mothers’ perception of child behavior and the affective-cognitive organizational characteristics of their childhood memories. Group I, The first group of hypotheses is about the mothers’ perception of child behavior. The first two hypotheses are specifically focussed on the deviancy categorization processes and will explore if the mothers’ of conduct-disturbed boys (MCDB) differ from the mothers’ of normal boys (MNB) in their definition and classification of deviant, neutral, and prosocial behaviors. The hypotheses will examine if the MCDB both label and track deviant and prosocial behaviors differently than the MNB. (The data are from the CBS, CBRSI, and CBRSII.) Hmthesis 1. The MCDB will be overly inclusive in their categorization of deviancy and overly exclusive (or underinclusive) in their categorization of prosocial behavior. The MCDB will have a greater tendency to perceive behaviors as deviant and will be less prone to define and identify behaviors as prosocial. Thus, as compared to the MNB, the MCDB will: (a) correctly identify a significantly greater number of deviant behaviors, (b) will incorrectly identify a significantly greater number of positive behaviors as negative, (c) will identify a significantly greater number of neutral behaviors as negative, (d) will have a "negative response 77 tendency", (i.e., out of all the behaviors identified, for each individual mother, a significantly greater proportion will be negative), and (e) the MCDB overemphasis on deviancy will also be reflected in their ratings of behaviors as significantly more aversive. The MNB will be more likely to perceive behaviors as prosocial and less likely to define and identify behaviors as deviant. These mothers will be overly inclusive in their categorization of prosocial behaviors and underinclusive (or exclusive) in their categorization of deviant behaviors. Thus, the MNB will (a) correctly identify a greater number of prosocial behaviors, (b) will incorrectly identify a greater number of negative behaviors as positive, (c) will identify a greater number of neutral behaviors as positive, (d) will have a "positive response tendency" (i.e., out of all the behaviors identified, for each individual mother, a significantly greater proportion will be positive), and (e) the MNB emphasis on prosocial behaviors will also be reflected in their ratings of behaviors as significantly more desirable. 78 The third hypothesis deals with the issues of whether there is a "behavior classification covariation" where an emphasis on deviancy would inherently result in a de-emphasis on prosocial behaviors and vice versa. WILL A mother’s emphasis in classifying behaviors as deviant will be related to a de-emphasis in classifying behaviors as prosocial and vice-versa. Hence, an over- inclusiveness in the categorization of deviancy will be related to an over-exclusiveness in the categorization of prosocial behaviors. The final hypotheses within this group will explore the association between the family’s level of enmeshment and the mother’s perceptual style, as well as examine related issues concerning the mother’s degree of engagement or involvement. Patterson (1982, 1986b) clearly characterized the MCDB as being overly enmeshed and very irritable. He described these mothers as being easily provoked and thus becoming quickly involved and engaged with the child. This high level of engagement or reactivity combined with the mother’s tendency to be overly inclusive in the classification of deviancy (thus frequently perceiving the child’s behavior as worthy of reprimand) results in recurrent aversive and negative mother-chfld interactions. (The data are from the CBS, CBRSI, CBRSII, and the FTQ.) Hymmesis 4. The MCDB will describe their current families as significantly more enmeshed than the description of the MNB current families. 79 W. The MCDB will identify a significantly greater number of behaviors, (i.e., both negative and positive). H i 6. The level of enmeshment in the family will be significantly related to the total number of behaviors identified. W. The level of enmeshment will be related to the mother’s deviancy-classification schema. 519.111.11- The second group of hypotheses, hypotheses 8 through 10, will examine the mother’s mental model regarding parent-child relationships, focussing on the ways in which she has mentally represented and organized her own childhood experiences. These predictions will explore not only the specific content of these childhood experiences but also the affective-cognitive organizational properties of these memories. (The hypotheses are based on the scores from the Adult Attachment Questionnaire (AAQ), and this includes the Preliminary Questions.) Hypothesis 8 will examine differences in the quality of the mother’s childhood relationships/experiences. H e is . The childhood recollections of the MCDB will exhibit (both directly expressed and inferred) a significantly greater amount of problematic and/or conflictual emotional experiences in childhood than the recollections of the MNB. The childhood recollections of the MCDB will exhibit a greater amount of Unloving, Rejection, 80 Neglect, Role-reversal, Pressure to Achieve, and Abuse than the recollections of the MNB. Hypothesis 9 involves establishing a relationship between the defensiveness of the mother’s recollections of her own childhood and the psychopathology of her child. Do the MCDB have a mental model regarding parent-child relationships that is more defensive in it’s orientation than the MNB? W2. The childhood recollections of the MCDB will be more defensively organized (i.e., Incoherent, ldealized, Lack of Detail (or specific recall), and Unresolved (or lack of resolution and/or separation-autonomy) than the recollections of the MNB. Hypothesis 10 will examine if the quality of the childhood experiences are related to the organizational properties of the maternal recollections. In other words: Is there a relationship between the "apparent" childhood experiences and the mothers’ current state of mind (i.e. , the affective-cognitive organizational properties) regarding these experiences? Hypgmesis 0. Problematic childhood experiences will be associated with childhood memories that are currently defensively organized. A defensive organization refers to the tendency to shift attention away from specific information regarding childhood relationships. Hence, problematic childhood experiences will be related to the 81 mothers’ tendency to distort, (i.e., Idealize), restrict (i.e., Lack of Detail) and splinter or disorganize, (i.e., Incoherent, and Unresolved) the information regarding these childhood experiences. QM. Hypothesis 11 represents the major thrust of the present study. It is an attempt to establish a link between the mother’s current perceptual style and the manner in which she has encoded or internally organized/represented her own childhood experiences. This hypothesis will explore if a mother’s deviancy categorization processes, (i.e., her current perceptual style) are related to the affecitve-cognitive organization of her childhood memories, (i.e., her mental model of parent-chfld relationships). HmthcsiLli. Mothers who are defensive in their organization of childhood memories (i.e., those that display a tendency to distort, restrict, and/or splinter or disorganize the information) will exhibit a corresponding need to shift attention away from positive, prosocial behaviors as these behaviors are assumed to be relationship oriented. In other words, this defensiveness or shift of attention away from relationship oriented information will result in a selective inattentiveness to prosocial behaviors and a subsequent over-emphasis (or selective attentiveness) to deviant behaviors. Thus, a mother’s defensiveness 82 regarding her own childhood will be related to her deviancy categorization processes (i.e., the greater the defensiveness, the more likely the mother will exhibit a categorization schema that is overly inclusive of deviant behaviors and overly exclusive of prosocial behaviors). CHAPTERZ METHOD Barium Only mothers were selected as participants in this study. It is reasonable to assume that mothers’ and fathers’ influences on a child’s development are not identical. This assumption is clearly supported in regards to the development of a child’s conduct disturbance (Hetherington & Martin, 1986; Patterson, 1982). For example, there are differences between mothers and fathers in terms of their perception of child deviance (Schaughency & Iahey, 1985) as well as differences in their responses to deviant behaviors (Patterson, 1982). Since there is support for the position that gender is an important characteristic involved in parenting (Frank, Jacobson, Hole, Justkowski, & Huyck, 1986; Perry, Wells, & Doran, 1983) only mothers were chosen. This choice is consistent with previous research reviewed in the introductory chapter. In the literature on conduct disorders (e.g., Anderson et al. , 1986; Forehand et al., 1975; Griest et al., 1980; Rickard et al.,1981) and the literature on parental perception (Furey & Forehand, 1986; Forehand et al., 1986; Rickard et al., 1984) mothers have been the primary participants. In fact, some studies referred to "parental" characteristics when actually they were examining only the mothers’ 83 84 characteristics (e. g., Rickard et al., 1984). Additionally, the preliminary research using the Adult Attachment Interview has been based primarily on mothers. While some of the previous research on conduct disorders (e. g. , Olweus, 1980; Patterson, 1982; Snyder, 1977) and parental perception has utilized both parents as participants (e.g., Christensen et al., 1983; Thompson & Bernal, 1982) in studies where only one parent participated, it was always the mother. Perhaps the best reason for having chosen mothers over fathers as participants in the present study is that the conceptual foundation underlying the hypotheses emphasizes the mothers’ influence on conduct disturbance and the coercive quality of the mother-son interactions (Hetherington & Martin, 1986; Patterson, 1982). The participants were 133 mothers of boys ranging in age between 6-11 years- old. On the basis of the mother’s responses during the initial phone interview, the mothers were divided into two groups: u i r e B DB The determination of a conduct disturbance was based primarily on each mother’s description of her son. As a group, this determination was supported by the sons’ scores on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). There were 71 mothers that described their sons during the initial phone interview as having behavior problems at home, either " fairly serious behavior problems" or "mild behavior problems". Correspondingly, the sons’ scores on the CBCL were, as a group, significantly higher than the comparable scores of the sons 85 in the "nonnal"/control group. (The CBCL scale scores and groups comparisons are presented in Table 2). Furthermore, the differences between the two groups on the Extemalizing Scales of the CBCL, and specifically Scale VIII, Aggressive, are particularly relevant in regards to the determination of a conduct disturbance. Scale VIII is the most important scale given that the items on this scale have many similarities to Patterson’s (1982) "Social Aggressors", and this type of conduct disturbance has also been referred to an " overt antisocial" pattern (Loeber & Schmaling, 1985 ; Patterson, 1982) since the child’s behavior is characterized by overtly confrontive antisocial acts which primarily involve an abrasive interaction with another person. (Table 3 lists the behaviors that make up these scales.) This pattern of conduct disturbance is highlighted by behaviors that are basically noncompliant, argumentative, aversive, and aggressive. For the MCDB, the sons’ mean score on the Aggressive Scale was 16.86 (SD = 7.70). Not only is this significantly higher than the control group, this mean score is also in the 95m percentile on the normative standardized scale (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). M rm B NB The determination of the son as "normal" (i.e., non-conduct-disturbed) was based primarily on the mother’s description of her son’s behavior. During the initial phone contact, there were 62 mothers that described their son’s behavior to be no more disruptive than the average boy. The mothers described their sons’ behavior as either "what you’d expect for his age" or as "very well behaved." The 86 Table2 LJ '- ir 1' Alo.hs r . Me s .. . r . oomari it Me; Alpha M DB MNBb 1. (49 M 5.12 Mu S. Schizoid .57 2.30 2.11 1.60 1.51 2.21' (126) Depressed .82 7.15 4.89 3.95 3.16 4.54" (121) Uncom: .77 4.00 3.00 2.63 2.27 2.99” (128) mumcatrve Obsessive- .77 5.42 4.36 3.31 3.47 3.06" (131) Compulsive Somatic .63 1.48 1.80 .94 1.37 1.98’ (131) Complaints Social .70 3.39 2.63 1.89 1.86 3.86“ (125) Withdrawal Hyperactive .79 6.30 4.05 3.19 2.76 5.21” (124) Aggressive .90 16.86 7.70 8.03 5.54 7.66" (126) Delinquent .77 3.00 2.60 .82 1.18 6.34” (100) Internal 20.35 12.96 12.42 8.82 4.17" (123) External 26.15 12.44 12.04 7.83 7.92" (119) Total 46.61 22.42 25.53 14.31 6.54" (120) Problems ‘I_r=7l. b11:62. ’p < .05, two-tailed. ”p < .01 , two-tailed. 87 Table 3 031-1 ot-w-g‘c eExte 'in .- fth CBCL Erratum: A s iv Mm Acts too young Argues Destroys own Can’t concentrate Brags things Hyperactive Confused Daydreams Destroys own things Impulsive Poor schoolwork Clumsy Prefers young kids Speech problem Cnrel to others Demands attention Disobeys at home Disobeys at school Poor peer relations Jealous Fights Lies, cheats Unliked Attacks people Screams Shows off Stubborn Moody Sulks Swearing Excess talk Teases Temper Threatens Loud Destroys others things Disobeys at school Bad friends Lies, cheats Runs away Sets fires Steals at home Steals outside home Swearing Truant Vandalism 88 mothers’ determination of their sons’ behaviors as within normal limits is corroborated by the scores on the CBCL. As a group, each CBCL scale score (with the exception of "depressed" which is in the high normal range) is within the lower realms of the standardized normal range. Additionally, as mentioned above, Table 2 illustrates that every scale score for the "normal" boys is significantly lower than those of the CBCL scores describing the conduct disturbed boys. E l . j! . l l The means and standard deviations of the demographic variables for both groups are shown in Table 4. Descriptive data were obtained for the following variables: the age of the sons, the age of the mothers and fathers, years of education for both the mothers and the fathers, family income, and the number of children in the family including the birth order of the son. As shown in Table 4 there are no statistical differences between the two groups on any of these demographic variables. Information was also obtained regarding the mother’s relationships to her son. In over 95% of the cases, the participant was the "natural" mother of the son. There were a few cases involving step-mothers and a few involving adoption, but there was no systematic differences between the groups, X2 (2, N = 133) = 1.76, p > .05. Step-mothers were included in the study as long as they had been living within the reconstructed family unit for a minimum of 3 years. This criterion was established in order to allow a sufficient amount of time for the step-mother to be involved as a parent and have an influence on the boy’s development. ID Table 4 D hi V l r M us It Gr om risons Variable: MCDB MNB L (at) M L Mm _ Son’s Age 8.64 1.56 8.32 1.85 1.05 (120) Mother’s Age 36.68 4.51 37.53 4.45 1.07 (124) Father’s Age 38.48 5.27 39.19 5.45 .75 (122) Mother’s 15.02 2.23 15.44 2.11 1.08 (130) Education‘ Father’s 15.99 2.73 16.18 2.85 .39 (126) Education‘ Family 58.12 32.31 51.38 27.36 1.30 (130) Income" Children in 2.64 1.03 2.62 1.70 .08 (97) Family F n V ' Chi-89m % of Firstboms 50.70 59.68 n.s. % of Middle child 26.76 19.35 n.s. # of Sons in Therapy 9 1 4,44‘ # of Sons with 24 8 7.06“ History of Therapy # of Mothers in 8 5 Therapy ‘In years. bIn thousands. ’p (.05, two-tailed. ”p <.01, two-tailed. 90 Finally, information was obtained regarding the family’s involvement in seeking treatment for psychological problems. Table 4 reveals that there were some significant differences in terms of seeking psychological intervention for the sons. There were a significantly greater number of conduct-disturbed boys currently involved in therapy (this included contacts with a school counselor), and a significantly greater number that had past involvement in treatment. These differences are understandable since the mothers of these boys perceive their child to have behavior problems and thus are more likely to seek professional intervention. P ' i 1 ' n The participants in this study were recruited from five school districts located within a 20 mile radius of Lansing, Michigan, the state capital. Lansing and the surrounding area has a population of approximately 200,000. Each of the respective school districts agreed to distribute a letter from the researcher to all of the mothers of all male students in their elementary schools (K through 6"I grade). The letter was sent home with the boys, usually as part of a weekly packet of school-related announcements. Four out of the five school districts wrote a brief endorsement and approval of the study. The school district’s letter appeared on the reverse side of the recruitment letter. Once the letter was distributed, the school districts had no other involvement with the research. The recruitment letter specifically asked those mothers that were interested in the study to contact the researcher directly in order to obtain more information. 91 A copy of the letter distributed to the mothers through the schools is included in the Appendix. The letter offered the mother free attendance to a parenting skills workshop in exchange for her participation in a study exploring important factors in parent-child interactions, especially those between mother and son. The letter attempted to locate "both mothers who consider(ed) their son to be a behavior problem in the home, as well as mothers who regard(ed) their son’s behavior to be no more disruptive than the average boy ". Those mothers not interested in the skills workshop were given the option to choose a $15.00 remuneration. Of the 133 participants, only 12 mothers chose the financial remuneration. There was no significant difference between the groups in terms of the mothers selecting the workshop or the financial remuneration, X’ (1, N = 133) = 3.16, 9 >05. The half-day workshop was described as being designed to help increase child management skills and to aid and offer some specific suggestions in dealing with their sons’ annoying and disturbing behaviors at home. Approximately 50 mothers eventually attended the parenting skills workshops which were held a few months after the completion of data collection. Linnaeus A variety of paper-and-pencil instruments were used in this research. Some are standardized instruments used in previous research and some were created and/or modified for the present study. The instruments used were: the Achenbach Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) the Family Information Sheet, 92 the Enmeshment Scale from Bloom’s (1985) Family Activities and Relationship Questionnaire, the Child Behavior Scenes, the Child Behavior Rating Scales, the Preliminary Questions, and the Adult Attachment Questionnaire. W "The Child Behavior Checklist is designed to obtain parents’ descriptions of their children’s behavior in a standardized format" (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983 , p.10). The parent is asked to rate 118 different behavior problems (e.g., "demands a lot of attention") on a three point scale. The parent rates the extent to which that behavior describes their own child (e.g., 0 = not at all, 1 = sometimes, and 2 = very often). Various syndromes of behavior problems were identified by principal component factor analyses of the CBCL items rated by parents of children referred for outpatient treatment. Relevant for the present study were the results for the 6-11 year—old boys. There were nine factors that emerged from their analyses. While the scales were developed on a clinical population in order to detect clinically significant syndromes, normative data have also been collected. Achenbach and Edelbrock (1983) report test-retest reliability of mothers’ rating for boys 6-11 years-old as follows: the Aggressive Scale was .95, the Delinquency scale was .95, and the Hyperactive scale was .92. The median one week test-retest correlation reliability for the mothers’ ratings for all scales at this age group was .76. All the above correlations are reported to be significant at p = .05 or less. In order to determine the suitability of the CBCL scales for the present sample, a reliability analysis was performed. Using the current sample, Table 2 shows the 93 Alphas calculated for each scale. The Alphas range between .57, for the Schizoid Scale, to .90, for the Aggressive Scale. Eamilxhrfsznuaticnfim This is a straightforward information sheet designed for the present study in order to obtain the participant’s demographic data. There are questions about family size, ages of family members, educational history of mother and father, mother’s relationship to son, and approximate family income. A copy of the Family Information Sheet is included in the Appendix. This is the Enmeshment Scale (i.e., Scale 15) from Bloom’s (1985) Family Activities and Relationship Questionnaire. The questionnaire is made up of 75-items that have been factor analyzed in four separate data collections/ studies. The analyses have consistently yielded 15 factors, or dimensions, of family functioning. The Enmeshment Scale, one of the 15 factors, is intended to assess "the extent to which family members are seen as insisting on interdependence to the exclusion of individual action" (p.237). It is a five item scale (e.g., "family members find it hard to get away from each other") and the mothers were asked to rate the extent to which each statement describes her current family (e. g., 1 = very untrue for our family, 2 = fairly untrue for our family, 3 = fairly true for our family, and 4 = very true for our family). Thus, the FT Q produced a score that ranged between 5-20. Bloom (1985) reported 94 norms for this scale as having a Mean = 9.5 and a SD. = 2.9. (A copy of the Family Togethemess Questionnaire is included in the Appendix.) Bloom (1985) concluded that there is satisfactory reliability for the Enmeshment Scale, reporting for each separate study the Cronbach Alpha at .72, .67 and .78, respectively. The average inter-item correlations on this Scale are reported to be .34, .30 and .42 for the three studies, respectively. On the other hand, an initial attempt at validation was unsuccessful as the Enmeshment Scale did not significantly differentiate intact families from families that subsequently were disrupted by divorce and separation (Bloom, 1985). This validation study, however, suffered from its reliance on retrospective data (i.e. , it was based on college students retrospective accounts of family life). A reliability analysis was performed on the FTQ for the present sample. The mean score was 9.15 (S1; = 3.09). The Alpha was .80, with the mean inter-item correlation = .46. hi1 vi r n The Child Behavior Scenes was developed specifically for the present study in order to examine parental perception of child behaviors; specifically, the parental tracking and labelling of positive and negative child behaviors. This instrument was designed to help test the hypotheses regarding differences in the mothers’ deviancy- categorization processes. The Child Behavior Scenes is comparable to both the methods (i.e., the stimuli and the procedures) and the goals of research reported by Holleran et al., (1982a, 1982b). 95 The scenes are four short scenarios depicting a variety of interactions primarily between boys and other family members; there are some peer interactions. The titles of the four scenes describe the context in which the child behaviors take place: After School, Getting Ready For Dinner, Dinner, and Around The House. Some of the behaviors for the fourth scene, Around The House, were suggested by Schmaling (1983). There are a total of 180 different child behaviors that are embedded in the four scenes. A behavior is defined by the use of a verb. The mothers were instructed to read through each of the four short stories and to underline with a red pen all those child behaviors that they considered negative, undesirable, aversive, hurtful, and/or inappropriate, and to underline with a green pen all those child behaviors that they considered positive, desirable, helpful, appropriate, or prosocial. The mothers were asked to imagine that it was their own son that was engaged in the behaviors that were depicted. When the Child Behavior Scenes was administered, the mothers were given a detailed instruction sheet which was read aloud by the researcher. The instruction sheet explained that the stories were designed to help study the different ways that parents follow or keep track of their children’s behaviors, including how parents vary in the way they judge different child behaviors. The instructions directed the mothers to read the stories carefully and to pay attention to what the boys do, what they think, how the boys react and respond, what they say, and how they feel. Specific examples were provided on the instruction sheet in order to help the mothers identify the 96 separate segments of child behaviors. (A copy of the instructions for the Child Behavior Scenes is included in the Appendix.) Each of the 180 behaviors were pre-experimentally classified as either a positive, negative, or neutral behavior in accordance with the following procedures: The 180 behaviors were listed in the same order in which they appeared in the stories, thereby providing the same contextual cues that the mothers bad. This list was then rated by 5 Clinical Psychologists, all psychotherapists. The therapists all have a Ph.D and had been in Clinical practice between 5-25 years; the mean number of years in practice was 14.4. The approximate percentage of their Clinical practice that involved working with children and families ranged between 30%-100% , (M = 58%). The therapists were asked to individually rate each of the 180 different child behaviors as either a positive, negative, or neutral behavior. The specific instructions to the therapists were as follows: For the negative behaviors: "Those behaviors that you judge to be clearly negative, inappropriate, hurtful, aversive, and/or undesirable please mark with an N, for Negative. " The instructions for the positive behaviors were: "Those behaviors that you judge to be clearly prosocial, appropriate, helpful, and/or desirable please mark with a I_’ for Positive." Finally, the instructions for judging a behavior as neutral were: "This will leave a variety of behaviors unmarked. The unmarked behaviors will be those that you cannot consider to be either clearly _N_egative or Positive. These behaviors may be neutral (i.e., they don’t seem either desirable or 97 undesirable) or they may be behaviors that you cannot clearly judge without more information regarding a specific context (i.e., they are ambiguous)" The final classification of these 180 child behaviors was based on the following criteria: A behavior was classified as positive if 4 out of the 5 of the Psychologists (i.e., 80%) judged the behavior to be positive. A behavior was classified as negative if 4 out of the 5 Psychologists (i.e., 80%) judged the behavior to be negative. The remaining behaviors were classified as "neutral" . In other words, those behaviors classified as neutral were the behaviors on the list that were either left unmarked by the therapists, and/or those behaviors that did not obtain the 80% criteria. There were 61 behaviors that reached the criteria to be classified as "positive". There were 42 behaviors that reached the criteria to be classified as "negative". The remaining 77 behaviors were classified as Neutral. (The final classification of each of the 180 behaviors that comprise the Child Behavior Scenes is presented in the Appendix.) The Child Behavior Scenes yielded a variety of scores for each participant: the number of correctly identified positive behaviors, the number of correctly identified negative behaviors, the number of positive behaviors identified as negative, the number of negative behaviors identified as positive, the number of neutral behaviors identified as positive, the number of neutral behaviors identified as negative, the total number of behaviors identified as negative, the total number of behaviors identified as positive and, the total number of behaviors identified. There is also a proportion 5C0 a :1 5 e1 13? WCI‘E and I focus: tgat‘ Iran 31cm 98 score (i.e., the proportion of behaviors identified as negative), which controls for each mother’s specific response rate. hi1 B vi r R ' e The Child Behavior Rating Scales were developed for the present study. They were designed to obtain the mothers judgments regarding the degree of aversiveness and the degree of desirability of specific child behaviors. Holleran et al., (1982a) reported using a similar procedure in examining the parental ratings of child behaviors in terms of the degree of aversiveness. The Child Behavior Rating Scales were divided into two sections. Section I focussed on the mothers’ ratings regarding the degree of misconduct of certain negative behaviors. Section II focussed on the mothers’ ratings of the desirability of certain positive behaviors. Section I of the Child Behavior Rating Scales is comprised of 63 statements or examples of child misconduct. The mothers were instructed to rate each statement on a six-point-scale. The mother’s ratings reflected her own judgment as to the seriousness of each misbehavior (e.g., 5 = an extremely serious misbehavior, 4 = serious misbehavior, both 2 and 3 = moderate, l = slight misbehavior and 0: not at all a misbehavior). Thus, the mothers’ scores on Section I, based on only 47 of these statements, could potentially range between 0-235 . The statements represent a variety of categories of misconduct: aggression, Stcalling, destruction of property, drug use, disobedience, deception, and allnoying/aversive social behaviors (e.g., insults, nagging, forgetfulness, and 99 inconsiderate of others). The variety of misbehavior ranges from the very extreme (e.g., "He took out a knife and threatened his teacher.") to the moderate (e. g., "He fed the dog an hour later than he was supposed to.") and includes some statements that might not be considered a misbehavior at all (e.g., "Stew reminded his parents that the movie started in 30 minutes"). (A copy of the Child Behavior Rating Scales I appears in the Appendix.) Section II of the Child Behavior Rating Scales is comprised of 63 statements or examples of prosocial child behaviors. The mothers were instructed to rate each statement on a six-point-scale. The mother’s ratings reflected her own judgment as to the desirability of each prosocial behavior (e. g., 5 = an extremely desirable behavior, 4 = highly desirable, both 2 and 3 = moderate, l = slightly desirable, and 0 = not a clearly desirable behavior). Thus, the mothers’ scores on Section II, based on only 53 of these statements, could potentially range between 0-265. The statements in Section 11 represent a variety of categories of prosocial behaviors: volunteerism, charity, cooperation, participation in groups and social activities, positive peer involvement, interests and hobbies, academic/intellectual pursuits, responsibility with money (e.g., earning and saving money) restraint of aggression, appropriate emotional expression (e.g. , appreciation and care) and honesty. The variety of prosocial behaviors ranges from extremely desirable (e. g., "Drake donated all of this weeks allowance to the fund for needy children") to moderately desirable (e.g., " He washed his hands for dinner."), and include some Statements that might not even be considered a prosocial behavior (e. g., "Ted put SONIC Ben Prior 4. linen itch I 56:90 on m, . (“pl Ilfms3 h! - n "all: \ 100 some more sugar in his chocolate drink."). (A copy of the Child Behavior Rating Scales II appears in the Appendix.) When the Child Behavior Rating Scales were administered, the mothers were given a detailed instruction Sheet. These instructions were read aloud by the researcher. The instruction sheet explained that the Child Behavior Scales were designed to study how parents judge certain behaviors. For both sections of the Child Behavior Rating Scales it was made clear that each behavior on the list was performed by a boy. The mothers were asked to imagine that the boy performing the behavior was the same age as her own son. Examples regarding the use of the scales and how to complete the ratings were provided. A principal Component Factor Analysis with a Varimax Rotation was performed on Section I and II, respectively, in order to determine the underlying dimensions measured by the instrument and to create more homogeneous scales. In each respective analysis one main factor emerged. For the Child Behavior Checklist Section I there were 47 items, from the 63 original items, that loaded .40 or higher on the one underlying dimension. The internal consistency of these 47 items that comprised Section I was measured by Alpha = .94. For Section II, there were 53 items, from the 63 original items, that loaded .40 or higher on the main factor. These 53 items comprising the scale for Section II had a Alpha = .95. This is a set of 10 questions that served as a summary of the mother’s relationships with her own parents. Five questions are focussed on the overall quality 3110 1011*! '1 I \_. a: T .4le ‘ A 101 of experience with the mother (e. g., loving, neglecting, rejecting, pushed to achieve, and role-reversing) and the same five questions are repeated in regards to the relationship with the father. For each question the respondent could choose one of five options that best reflected her own experiences (e.g., in describing how loving the relationships were with her parents, the mother could choose between very lacking in love, lacking in love, neither loving nor actively unloving, loving, and very loving). The Preliminary Questions were designed specifically for the present study as an instrument to be used in conjunction with the Adult Attachment Questionnaire (see below). The Preliminary Questions were administered up to four weeks prior to the administration of the Adult Attachment Questionnaire. These quickly obtained summary statements were subsequently either supported or contradicted by the specific content of the experiences recalled in the detailed responses to the full length questionnaire. Thus, the Preliminary Questions do not yield a specific score, but rather they functioned as an aid in scoring the organizational properties of the mother’s presentation of attachment-related information, particularly in terms of its overall coherency and consistency. (A copy of the Preliminary Questions can be found in the Appendix.) A l A n ' ' AA The Adult Attachment Questionnaire is an open-ended questionnaire that asked the mothers to describe their childhood relationships with their parents and to give memories supportive of these descriptions. Specific questions were asked about 1110 11.11: 1:98 mil ital quest Semi- 102 feelings of rejection, experiences of being upset, hurt, or ill, and separations. The mothers were also asked about their own understanding of why their parents acted the way they did, the effects of these early relationships on their adult personality, the influence of other significant relationships with adults, and the current status of the mother’s relationships with her parents, including changes over the years. The AAQ is based on the Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985). (An original version of the AAQ was designed by Hazan & Shaver (1988) but has not yet been used in research. The current version is a slight modification, discussed below, of their efforts.) The AAQ is an attempt to obtain attachment-relevant information (i.e. , regarding parent-chfld relationships) in a questionnaire format that is comparable to the information obtained from the 1-2 hour semi-structured interview. In fact, the specific questions from the interview are used as the items on the questionnaire. Fifteen of the 18 questions that comprise the interview do appear on the AAQ, with an added question regarding the elicitation of parental attention. In essence, the same basic questions are asked, but the ability to probe and ask for clarification is obviously not part of the questionnaire format. Other minor changes from the Hazan and Shaver (1988) version of the AAQ have been made in terms of wording and formatting. These changes include the addition of the inquiry on the elicitation of parental attention, as well as the elimination of the i"quiry into significant losses during childhood; (issues of unresolved grief were not explored in the current research.) (A copy of the AAQ is included in the Appendix.) 'sm: prom quest: m, P C“ '015 Mm; 103 The Adult Attachment Rating and Classification System (Main & Goldwyn, 1989) provided the basis for the scoring system that was used to score the AAQ. This scoring system was originally designed for use with the Adult Attachment Interview. It is a scoring system for assessing and ultimately classifying an adult’s "state of mind with respect to attachment" (Main & Goldwyn, 1989, p. 2). Each protocol is given several nine—point ratings on various scales. Then, based on the patterning of these scale scores, each protocol is assigned a single overall classification representing the individual’s internal working model (or present state of mind) with respect to attachment. A pilot study using the AAQ was undertaken in order to determine the questionnaire’s feasibility and usefulness as a data collection procedure and to assess the applicability of the Adult Attachment Rating and Classification System. Thus, the pilot study provided the data necessary in the development/modification of the scoring manual for the specific use with the AAQ (i.e., verifying which of the previously used scales were also scoreable given the format of the AAQ) as well as providing the protocols necessary in the training of raters for the present study. The participants in this pilot study were 50 undergraduates from a large mid- westem university who were enrolled in an introductory Psychology class. Participation in research was part of the course requirements. The students were told that this was a pilot study aimed at developing a questionnaire that would explore Childhood memories. The Preliminary Questions and the AAQ were both administered at the same time and in a group Situation. mum .ilQ score: 0kg Sales Mei Pitta l .Iane‘ imam "3 30ml] 104 On the basis of the data/results from this pilot study using the AAQ, the differences between the AAQ and the Attachment Interview, and the differences in the conceptual focus between the present study and the original work of Main and Goldwyn (1989), slight changes and modifications in both the content of the AAQ and the original scoring system were implemented. In modifying the scoring manual for the AAQ every effort was made to maintain the conceptual integrity, spirit, and nuances of the Adult Attachment Rating and Classification System. The actual changes included: the development and/or elimination of specific questions on the AAQ, the development of new variables/scales, the elimination of certain scale scores, the establishment and/or modification of new criteria used for the assignment of specific scale scores, the modification of the original nine-point scales to five-point scales, and the use of only certain rating scales in the present study (i.e., the participants’ overall attachment—classification was not determined since it was not a particularly relevant issue for the present study). The AAQ yielded 12 scores; 10 scale scores and two composite scores. The ratings for each scale/variable were based on the information obtained primarily from the entire AAQ protocol and, to a limited extent, from the information obtained from the Preliminary Questions. An essential feature of the scoring system is that the entire protocol is taken into account rather than any single answer in isolation. Hence, the assignment of scale scores were not based on single conclusive statements in the protocols; rather, the raters were directed to look at the total picture, looking for contradictions and unsupported statements. For example, the choice of " not at all ti, 3312‘ e ‘h l slljdk“ .L1 to Mint [:0 5F 105 rejecting", exhibited either on the Preliminary Questions or initially presented in the questionnaire, may still be part of a protocol scored for rejection on the basis of either a lack of supportive evidence and/or on the basis of contradictory content. Six of the ten scales are experiential, representing the mother’s "inferred" childhood experiences. This is in accordance with Main and Goldwyn’s (1989) caution that these scales represent "the probable experiences of each parental figure during childhood" (p. 2). The six scale scores regarding the mother’s probable childhood experiences are: Loving/Unloving, Rejection, Neglect, Role-Reversing/ Involving, Pressure to Achieve, and Abuse. For each of these six scales, separate ratings were given to the subject’s experiences with mother and father during childhood. Brief descriptions of the six scales assessing the mother’s probable childhood experiences are as follows: Lgvinglflnlgving. This scale assesses the extent to which the participant seems to have experienced her mother or father as loving or unloving during childhood. The scale is designed to assess whether the subject had a firm sense of the emotional support and availability of the parent, especially in times of trouble. Rejecfion. This scale assesses the extent to which the parent rejected and/or avoided the child’s moves towards dependence and attachment. The scale is specifically focussed on that rejection which aimed at shifting the child’s attention away from feelings of attachment. "The essential quality being assessed as rejection is the quality of turning back or (turning) away of the 106 child’s dependence, affection, attention, need and attachment." (Main & Goldwyn, 1989, p.18) Neglect. This scale assesses the extent to which the parent was physically and psychologically unavailable, unresponsive, or inaccessible. It is a rating of a parent that is uninvolved, preoccupied, otherwise occupied, inattentive, or absent. Role-Reversing/Invglving. This scale assesses the extent to which the parent made him/herself an object of the child’s attention, involving the child in his/her physical or psychological care, and/or needing the child’s involvement for the maintenance of his/her security, everyday functioning, and well-being. W. This scale assesses the extent to which the child was pressured to achieve some particular Status or position. It goes beyond encouragement to do well, but rather the rating is based on the extent to which the child’s attachment and needs were responded to only in connection with specific achievements or accomplishments. Abuse. This scale is intended to rate the degree of physical, sexual and/or emotional abuse that the participant experienced during childhood. This scale assesses the extent to which the parental response or behavior is inappropriate, extreme, excessive, and exhibits an overall disregard for the child’s well being and individuality; at the extreme, the basic parental attitude is one of contempt towards the child. 107 This Abuse scale was not part of the Main and Goldwyn (1989) scoring system but was added because it seemed like a potentially important aspect of one’s childhood experiences. Wm. This variable is a composite score of the six inferred-experiences scales (i.e., Overall Problematic Emotional Relations = Unloving + Rejection + Role-Reversal + Neglect + Pressure to Achieve + Abuse.) The composite score is taken as an overall index of the mother’s emotional difficulties encountered as a child in regards to attachment- related experiences. The AAQ yielded another five scores or variables that were intended to assess the mother’s present state of mind with respect to attachment (Main & Goldwyn, 1989). These variables refer to the mental representation of attachment-related experiences and to the organization of the information relevant to these experiences. Hence, these scales, (there are four scales and one composite score), assess the ease with which information is accessed, the apparent distortion which certain information has undergone, the overall organization of attachment-related information, and to specific feelings. As Main and Goldwyn ( 1989) point out, each of these scales "deal with the current representation of the self in relationship to attachment figures, attachment-related experiences and attachment-related phenomena" (p.40). The following "present state of mind" scales were used: Incoherent. This scale reflects the extent to which the mother is able to present and assess her past and its influences in a fluid, cohesive, and consistent 108 manner; a coherent protocol is easily understandable and permits or suggests few or no alternative interpretations to those already offered by the participant herself. Main and Goldwyn (1989) do point out that this variable stands as the over- riding assessment of an individual’s state of mind with respect to attachment. In previous research, Coherency has accounted for the largest amount of variance in the dependent variables (e. g, Main & Goldwyn, 1984). Idealizafion. This scale assesses the extent to which the mother retains some image of a "perfect" parent, separated from actual experience and held separate from actual experience. The scale assesses the discrepancy between the overall viewpoint of the parent (e. g., on the Preliminary Questions) and the specific content of the attachment-related experiences. W. This scale assesses the actual amount and quality of memory (i.e. , specific content) that is demonstrated within the protocol. This scale assesses the extent to which the subject provides actual memories and recalls specific events and circumstances of their own childhood. The scale assesses the degree of specificity of the recollections, rating the richness/depth of the historic content that is contained in the written protocol. This scale, developed for the current research, is intended to be a memory scale assessing information availability or information accessibility. It is loosely based on the Main and Goldwyn (1989) scale named "Frequency and Strength of Insistence Upon Inability to Recall Childhood". Main and Goldwyn (1989) point out 1'11 qua 78$; aft: Tiem 109 that it is not possible to assess the subject’s actual inability to recall childhood. Therefore, Main and Goldwyn (1989) rate the frequency and strength of the mother’s insistence upon her inability to recall childhood. The Inability to Recall scale is an assessment based on the amount of times and the degree of insistence that the interviewee states she can’t remember. The primary reason that this Inability to Recall scale was determined inappropriate with the AAQ involves the differences in format between the questionnaire versus the actual interview. The pilot data revealed that references to one’s own memory, and specifically the disclaimer of "I can’t remember", is not a response used with sufficient frequency in written protocols; it apparently does have a frequent usage within the verbal discourse of the interview. Thus the present scale, rather than being based on the subject’s reflection and self-appraisal regarding their own ability to remember childhood, is based on the actual amount and quality of memory (i.e., the specific content) that is demonstrated within the protocol. Unresolved; Seoeeao'on god Autonomy Seale, This scale assesses the extent or degree to which childhood relationships, especially difficult emotional experiences, have been understood, resolved, and integrated. The scale assesses not only a cognitive awareness and/or understanding of the influences (both positive and negative) of one’s own childhood experiences on adult relationships and functioning, but also the extent to which the participant had achieved an emotional distance, separateness, and psychological independence from her childhood relationships. 110 This scale was developed for the current research. Data from the pilot study suggested that a participant’s " state of mind" regarding attachment-related experiences is significantly influenced by the degree to which childhood conflicts have been worked-through and resolved. Main and Kaplan (1984) present evidence supporting the importance of a Resolution of Childhood variable, suggesting that it is not the quality of the mother’s childhood attachments per se, but rather it is the extent to which she has come to terms and resolved her past that is of critical importance. Relatedly, Main and Goldwyn (1989) make a distinction regarding the quality of the affect represented and exhibited by the participants. In their Anger scale they differentiate between an "involved/pre-occupi " anger versus an "autonomous" form. In the pro-occupied expression the subject expresses anger towards the parent which indicates current involvement in feelings of anger and an emotionally charged derogation of the parent (i.e. , a lack of separation and resolve). Similarly, the current Unresolved scale assesses not only the relevant cognitive representations (i.e., the self appraisals) but also takes into account the quality of the affects that are represented. Totfl Defensiveness. This is a composite score for the "state of mind" variables; (Total Defensiveness = Incoherence + Idealization + Lack of Detail + Unresolved). This composite score is taken as an overall index of the defensiveness of the mother’s cognitive model regarding her own childhood experiences. 3 I» r‘ 111 Raters The AAQ was scored and rated by five raters; four were graduate students in Clinical Psychology and one was a Clinical Social Worker. Half-way through the actual scoring of the data one rater withdrew because of time constraints. While the raters volunteered to participate primarily to familiarize themselves with Main and Goldwyn’s Adult Attachment Rating and Classification System, they were also financially compensated $10.00 per protocol. The raters were not aware of the specific hypotheses being tested and they were blind as to group membership of the participants whose protocols they were scoring. liainiog. The raters all met together with the researcher for approximately 20 hours of training. The training was conducted primarily on the pilot data, with the very end of training involving actual participant protocols. Training emphasized familiarization and learning of the scoring manual. There were 12 protocols that each rater scored independently over the course of the training. The raters would meet as a group with the researcher, compare scores and discuss the protocols. These discussions, which accounted for the bulk of the training, were focussed on points of disagreement. Disagreements between the raters appeared to be the result of carelessness, ambiguities in the scoring system, the complexity of the data and the scoring system, and personal biases. Due to the very personal and subjective nature of the protocols, clinical judgments were susceptible to biases, distortions, and "transference-type" reactions, similar to those experienced in a therapeutic setting. These issues were addressed as needed. Finally, the training 112 allowed the raters to acquire a familiarity with the potential range of data (e.g., between a very coherent protocol versus a strikingly incoherent protocol) as prototypical examples were provided. Determination of Inger—Raier Reliability. The statistic used to determine inter- rater reliability was the percentage of agreement-within-one. Thus, for example, a protocol assigned a score of "3" for Incoherent was considered to be in agreement if another rater scored the same protocol and scale a "2." The rationale for using this as an index of reliability is derived from the very nature of the data. The data are full of complexity, subtlety, and nuance. It is a questionable undertaking to assume you can assign numbers/scores to describe an event as intricate as one’s childhood relationships. Inherent in the very nature of this task is a heavy reliance on subjective clinical judgement. While there may be clear differences between a score of "1" and a score of "3", the difference on some protocols between a score of "3" and a score of "2" can be so elusive as to be meaningless. Clearly, this is not an exact science and a significant amount of measurement error, no matter how determined the effort, is to be expected. Thus, for present purposes "agreement-within—one" will be considered essential agreement. As mentioned above, each of the five raters, respectively, independently scored all twelve of the protocols used in the training. This subsequently allowed for many pairwise comparisons in order to determine inter-rater reliability as each individual judgement could be compared with that of the four other raters. A minimum criterion level of 80% agreement-within—one was considered acceptable. Table 5 presents the 1 13 Table 5 Pr mnB eenRter for eDurin R rT inin _R_a_te_r_s I II III IV V % of Exact Agreement‘ 50 52 57 50 57 % of Agreement Within-one‘ 88 90 92 91 93 Scale; m % Agreement wimio-one Unloveb 52 91 Rejectionb 49 88 Neglectb 59 95 Achieveb 63 95 Role-Reversalb 50 85 Abuseb 69 94 Incoherentc 36 83 Idealizationc 52 86 Recallc 51 90 Unresolvec 40 90 ‘Based on 782 pairwise comparisons. bBased on 270 pairwise comparisons. cBased on 135 pairwise comparisons. nut. fie as. I?» 114 inter-rater agreement across the twelve training protocols. At the end of training the inter-rater agreement ranged between 83 % agreement within-one (for the Incoherence scale) and 95% agreement within-one (for both the Pressure to Achieve and Neglect scales). In order to determine if this acceptable level of inter-rater agreement within- one was being maintained during the actual scoring of the data, two different reliability checks were undertaken. The initial reliability check was shortly after training was completed and the first 20 protocols had been scored. The second reliability check was performed about half-way through the scoring, after 60 protocols had been completed. For both of these reliability checks ten protocols were used, each protocol was scored by two raters; hence, each rater was compared once with every other rater. Table 6 presents the combined data from these two reliability checks. All scales, except for Idealization, maintained an adequate level of inter-rater agreement. The Idealization scale was at 90% agreement- within-one at the first check but this level of agreement was not maintained. Proeedure The experimental procedure was very straightforward. It can be divided into three segments: (a) the initial phone interview; (b) questionnaires completed at home, and (c) the three instruments administered during the data collection session. 115 Table 6 r f In r-R r A r m n -Within-On for R rs d Sc Ratere Scale I II III IV V Scaiclmal Unlove 100 88 94 94 88 93 Rejection 100 88 100 100 88 95 Neglect 81 94 75 88 100 88 Role- 100 88 94 88 94 93 Reversal Achieve— 88 88 88 88 88 88 ment Abuse 100 94 100 94 100 98 Incoherent 100 75 100 88 88 90 Idealiz- ation 88 63 63 88 75 75 Recall 100 88 88 100 75 9O Unresolve 100 100 100 88 88 95 Rater’s 95 87 91 92 91 Total Note. The scales totals for the first six scales are based on 40 pairwise comparisons; the remaining four "state of mind" variables are based on 20 pairwise comparisons. The rater’s totals are based on 128 pairwise comparisons. 116 As requested in the letter sent home through the schools, the mother’s first contact with the researcher was over the telephone. This initial phone contact consisted of a ten minute interview. (The outline of the phone interview and the Initial Phone Contact sheet completed during the conversation are in the Appendix.) The goals of the phone interview were: (a) to establish contact/rapport; (b) to determine the conduct-disturbed versus non-conduct—disturbed status of the mother’s son; (c) to obtain some demographic information; (d) to determine the mother’s choice of either the parenting skills workshop or the financial remuneration, and (e) to begin tentative scheduling for the data collection session. At the completion of the phone interview, it was explained that a packet would be mailed to them within a week. This packet contained a letter of introduction with an explanation of their participation in the study, a consent form, the CBCL, the Family Togethemess Questionnaire, and the Preliminary Questions. The total packet took the mothers approximately 30-40 minutes to complete. The mothers were asked to return the completed packet within 7 to 10 days in a postage paid envelope. The participants were then contacted by phone and scheduled for the 3-4 hour research session. A week prior to the data collection a letter confirming time and place was sent to the mother. During the 48 hours preceding the session a follow-up phone call was made for final confirmation. At the research sessions all the instruments were administered in a group setting. Both MCDB and MNB participated at the same time. The average size 117 group was about fifteen mothers. Upon arrival and check-in with the researcher, the mother was asked to complete the Family Information Sheet. After everyone had arrived, the mothers were formally welcomed to the data collection session; a brief overview of the research as well as the agenda for the 3-4 hour session was provided. The Child Behavior Scenes was then administered. After the entire group had completed this instrument both sections of the Child Behavior Rating Scales were administered. This was followed by a 10-15 minute break, at which time refreshments were provided. Following the break, the AAQ was administered. This left approximately two and a half hours to complete the AAQ. After 75 minutes the researcher announced the time and he suggested how much time was left. Practically all the mothers completed the AAQ between 1.5-2.5 hours. As the individual participants completed the AAQ and turned in their completed data packet, they were thanked for their time, given a debriefing sheet, and allowed to depart. Those mothers who choose the financial remuneration were paid at this time. CHAPTER3 RESULTS This chapter will be organized into four sections. The first three sections correspond to the three groups of hypotheses; namely, ( 1) those hypotheses that examine the differences in the mothers’ deviancy categorization processes; (2) those hypotheses that explore differences in the mother’s recollections of her childhood experiences and (3) the hypothesis that examines the relationship between the mother’s perception of child behavior and the affective-cognitive organizational characteristics of her childhood memories. ‘ The final section of this chapter will present data from an analysis performed on a sub-sample of the original sample. The rationale for the sub-sample and the results of this analysis will be presented in the fourth section of this chapter. Green I; Meterng Pergotion of Chile Behavior This group of hypotheses explores differences in the mothers’ perception of child behaviors, specifically their deviancy categorization processes. Hyoomesi 1. As can be seen in Table 7, there is general support for this hypothesis with 4 out of the 6 comparisons yielding statistically significant results. 118 119 Table 7 "A9L ii . ‘ ' M DB .Vl'l 1“] IV in ' 1:". . -|.Ol'l Of DCVi- ve x 1 ive in Th ir ti n of Proso ial Beh vior? Comparison: MCDB m 1. The MCDB correctly identified a M 36.79 35.24 significantly greater number of negative S_D_ 4.51 5.71 behavioral segments than did the MNB. t (131) = 1.74’ 2. There was not a significant difference M .36 .38 between the MCDB and the MNB in terms SD .63 .81 of the number of prosocial behavioral I(114) = .16 n.s. segments judged incorrectly as negative. 3. The MCDB judged a significantly greater M 9.75 8.26 number of neutral behavioral segments to be E 4.52 4.62 deviant than did the MNB. 1(131) = 1.88' 4. Overall, the MCDB identified a greater M 47.61 44.61 number of behaviors as negative than did the S_ 7.99 9.44 MNB. §(131) = 1.98" 120 Table 7 continued Comparisons 5. Within each mother’s specific response rate, the MCDB identified a significantly greater proportion of behaviors as negative than did the MNB.‘ 6. The MCDB did not rate behaviors as significantly more aversive than the ratings of the MNB. M DB MNB M 40.78 38.13 5;) 7.28 6.15 i(131) = 2.25" M 132.67 126.67 52 23.26 25.10 3(131) = 1.43 n.s. Note. Comparisons 1-5 are based on data from the CBS. Data for comparison 6 comes from the CBRSI. ‘This is a percentage score calculated, for each mother, as follows: (Total number of behaviors identified as negative / Total number of behaviors identified, (i.e. , as either positive or negative» x 100. The score describes the mother’s response tendency. 'o < .05, one-tailed. "o < .05,two-tai1ed. 121 As compared to the MNB, the MCDB: (a) correctly identified a significantly greater number of negative behaviors; (b) judged a significantly greater number of neutral behaviors to be negative; and (c) out of the 180 total behaviors (i.e. , positive, negative, and neutral), the MCDB identified a significantly greater number of the behaviors to be negative. Finally, for each mother, her " negative response tendency" was calculated, i.e., (the mother’s total # of behaviors identified as negative / the mother’s total number of all behaviors identified) x 100. Thus, for each mother this was a percentage of her specific responses that were negative. This response tendency was significantly more negative for the MCDB. While the data support the position that the MCDB are more likely to perceive behaviors as negative, it does not, however, support the prediction that MCDB are conversely less prone to define and identify behaviors as prosocial. There was no difference between the groups of mothers in the number of positive behaviors incorrectly identified as negative, (a judgement that was very rare in this sarnple) and, even though the MCDB did rate behaviors as more aversive than the MNB, the differences in their ratings was not statistically significant. W. As can be seen in Table 8, there is little support for the predictions that the MNB are more likely to perceive behaviors as prosocial and less likely to identify behaviors as deviant. The MNB were not different from the MCDB in terms of (a) the number of correctly identified prosocial behaviors; (b) the number of negative behaviors perceived as positive; (c) the number of neutral behaviors 122 Table 8 i 2' Ar M v r1 Inl iv in Ca riza'on f Prosoi v x 1v Th H n vi ‘7 Catamarans. M DB MNB 1. The number of prosocial behaviors correctly identified by the MNB was not significantly different from those identified by the MCDB. 2. The was no difference between the MCDB and MNB in their tendency to incorrectly identify negative behavioral segments as prosocial. 3. The MNB did not identify a significantly greater number of neutral behavioral segments to be prosocial behaviors than did the MCDB. 4. Overall, the number of behaviors identified as positive was not significantly different for the MCDB as compared to the MNB. M 49.45 51.02 S 9.30 9.95 1(131) = .94 n.s. IE .97 1.34 E 1.91 1.76 ;(l3l) = 1.15 n.s. IZ 21.73 22.53 SD 13.15 12.76 t(131) = .35 n.s. IZ 72.18 74.89 21.65 21.67 If!) U i(13l) = .72 n.s. 1 23 Table 8 continued 5. Within each mother’s specific response rate, the MNB identified a significantly greater proportion of behaviors as positive than did the MCDB.‘ 6. The MNB did not rate behaviors as significantly more desirable than the ratings of the MCDB. M DB MNB M 59.22 61.86 SD 7.28 6.15 1(131) = 2.25" M 188.17189.13 S 30.41 34.81 1(131) = .17 n.s. Mote. Comparisons 1-5 are based on data from the CBS. Data for comparison 6 comes from the CBRSI. ‘This is a percentage score calculated, for each mother, as follows: (Total number of behaviors identified as positive / Total number of behaviors identified, (i.e. , as either positive or negative» x 100. The score describes the mother’s response tendency. ”o < .05 ,two-tailed. 124 judged to be positive; and ((1) out of the 180 total behaviors, there was no difference in the number of behaviors judged to be positive. Also, the MNB did not rate behaviors as any more desirable than the same behaviors rated by the MCDB. The only support for this hypothesis is that the " response tendency" of the MNB was significantly more positive than the MCDB. This percentage score, however, is the inverse of the " negative response tendency" score derived in Hypothesis 1. W1. Table 9 shows the intercorrelations between all of the perceptual variables derived from the CBS, the CBRSI and CBRSII, and the FT Q. There is no support for the position that there is a "behavior classification covariation" where an emphasis on deviancy would inherently result in a de-emphasis on prosocial behaviors and vice versa. In fact, while the a priori predictions were in terms of inverse relationships, the results indicate positive relationships, with 3 out of the 4 relevant correlations revealing significant positive relationships. As can be seen in Table 9, there is a significant relationship between the number of neutral segments judged to be positive and the number judged to be negative, I. (133) = .37, o <.001. There is a significant relationship between the number of correctly identified positive behaviors and the number of correctly identified negative behaviors, I (133) = .53, o < .001. And, there is a significant relationship between the mother’s ratings of aversiveness and her ratings of desirability, ; (133) = .40, o < .001. There is not a relationship between the number of positive behaviors incorrectly judged as negative and the number of negative behaviors incorrectly judged as positive, ; (133) = .10, o > .05. l 25 Table 9 22mm mole l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 .NegNeg‘ .NegPos" .18“ .NegNeut‘ .51" .21* . PosPos‘ .53** .05 .34“ .PosNeg‘ -.23* .10 -.02 .12 .PosNeut‘ .35" .12 .37“ .74“ -.33** .NegPro‘ .06 .05 .15* -.74** -.39** -.72** .CBRSI'I .15“ .07 .29“ -.13 .00 -.01 .27" .CBRSIIi -.13 .03 .04 -.02 .19* .15* -.09 .40“ 10.l'~'l’Qi -.01 -.04 .02 .11 .08 .16“ -.l6* -.08 -.01 ”Response" .63" .18* .57“ .89“ .23“ .91" -.60** .02 .05 .13 \OOOQGUI-bWNr— Note. All the correlations are based on u = 133 ‘# of Negative statements correctly identified. "# of Positive statements judged to be negative. °# of Neutral statements judged to be negative. dtil of Positive statements correctly identified. °# of Negative statements judged to be Positive. ’# of Neutral Statements judged to be positive. gProportion of negative responses. 1‘The mothers’ ratings of the degree of negativity of negative behaviors. ’The mothers’ ratings of the degree of desirability of prosocial behaviors. JT he Family Togethemess Questionnaire is an index of the family’s enmeshment. kResponse is the total number of behaviors identified on the CBS and is taken as an index of the mother’s responsivity. ’o < .05, one-tailed. "o < .01, one-tailed. 126 Table 10 R ' nshi B e n Enme hment Mother’s Devianc -Classifi a 'on Sch m Correlation wim M11}. Enmeshmen PHVBI. l. E 1i 1' Total # of Behaviors Identified .126 # of Neutral Behaviors Judged to be Negative .020 # of Negative Behaviors Correctly Identified .012 # of Prosocial Behaviors Judged to be Negative -.036 Mother’s Ratings of Behaviors as Aversive -.082 Mother’s Response Tendency--Proportion of Total # of Behaviors Identified Judged to be Negative -. 163’ Inv R ' 11 hi Pr icted° # of Neutral Behaviors Judged to be Prosocial .159’ # of Prosocial Behaviors Correctly Identified .109 # of Negative Behaviors Judged to be Prosocial .079 Mother’s Ratings of Behaviors as Desirable -.012 ‘fl = 133 ’o < .05 in her :‘tSp a 512 511m: A. 127 W. This set of hypotheses explores the association between the family’s level of enmeshment and the mother’s perceptual style. (1' able 9 is relevant for Hypothesis 6 and 7.) While the MCDB did describe their families (on the FTQ) as significantly more enmeshed M = 9.69, SD = 3.41) than the descriptions of the MNB M = 8.53, SD = 2.57), t (128) = 2.23, o < .05, two- tailed, they did not identify (i.e., level of responsiveness) a greater number of total behaviors, (both positive and negative combined), M = 119.79, SD = 25.71) than the total number of behaviors identified by the MNB M = 119.50, SD = 28.37), 1(131) = .06, [2 >05. As can be seen in Table 10, Hypotheses 6 and 7 are not supported, as there is virtually no relationship between the mother’s deviancy-classification schema (i.e. , her perceptual style) and her description of her family as enmeshed. As seen in Table 10, there is not a significant relationship between enmeshment and the "mother’s responsivity" (i.e., the total number of behaviors identified). Even though there is a significant inverse relationship between the mother’s negative response tendency and enmeshment, and a significant positive relationship between the number of neutral behaviors identified as positive and enmeshment, both of these relationships, however, are not in the direction predicted a priori. Finally, Table 9 indicates that no other significant relationships exist between enmeshment and any of the remaining perceptual variables. The results for this group of seven hypotheses can be summarized as follows: There is general support for Hypothesis 1 as the MCDB do have a greater tendency 128 to perceive behaviors as deviant; but contrary to the predictions, they are not "less prone to define and identify behaviors as prosocial". Hypothesis 2 is not supported as there is virtually no difference between the groups in terms of their perception of prosocial behaviors (i.e. , the MNB were not more likely to perceive behaviors as prosocial). Furthermore, Hypothesis 3 is not supported as an emphasis in classifying behaviors as deviant is m related to a de-emphasis in classifying behaviors as prosocial. In support of Hypothesis 4, the MCDB did describe their current families as more enmeshed. This variable, however, is not related to the other perceptual variables. For example, the MCDB did not identify a greater number of behaviors, as predicted in Hypothesis 5, and the mothers’ level of responsivity (i.e., the number of behaviors identified) was not related to level of enmeshment as predicted in Hypothesis 6. Finally, Hypothesis 7 is not supported as there is virtually no relationship between the level of enmeshment and the mothers’ deviancy-classification schema. ' M rn hildh Recollections Hypotheses 8-10 are focussed on the mother’s recollections of her childhood relationships. The analysis explores not only the specific content of these childhood experiences, but also the organizational properties of these memories (i.e., the mother’s mental model). The data are primarily from the AAQ and only minimally from the Preliminary Questions. 129 Table 11 u or or 3' "r - DB . d : MNB n - ontent ofTheir Childhod Recollections mm M DB MNB 1‘. L411 Unloving M 7.14 7.18 .13 (131) SD 2 04 1.97 Rejection M 6.34 6.59 .73 (131) SD 2.02 1.97 Neglect M 4.51 4.37 .40 (131) SD 2.17 1.91 Role—Reversal M 4.77 5.18 1.19 (131) 52 2.01 2.01 Pressure to Achieve M 3.95 4.53 1.59 (131) SQ 1.89 2.33 Abuse M 4.93 4.82 .31 (131) so 2.16 2.00 Total Problematic Relations M 31.66 32.69 .74 (131) Si; 8.35 7.72 Note. All of the scales were scored in the same direction with higher scores indicating greater problematic experiences. ‘For all of the 115 reported, o > .05. 130 Table 12 91101:. on. B een ‘ M DB .. . ' MNB on e Affec'v- o'ni-v Pr ' f Th ir hildh R c lle 'on Scales. MCDB MNB _I: am Incoherent M 3.58 3.29 1.73' (131) S_12 .91 .97 Unresolve M 3.57 3.27 1.88’ (131) 112 .85 .99 Idealized M 2.58 2.54 .24 (131) SQ 1.12 1.06 Lack of Recall M 3.10 2.81 1.67’ (131) SD .96 1.05 Total Defensiveness M 12.84 11.91 1.75' (131) Si; 2.81 3.28 Note. All of the scales were scored in the same direction with higher scores indicating greater affective-cognitive disorganization or defensiveness. ‘M = 133. ’o < .05, one-tailed. 131 As seen in Table 11, the MCDB did not produce memories that exhibited a significantly greater amount of problematic emotional experiences. For each specific scale, as well as for the Total Problematic Relations score, the childhood recollections of the MCDB were not Significantly different in content than the memories recalled by the MNB. On the other hand, as can be seen in Table 12, Hypothesis 9 is generally supported as the affective-cognitive organizational properties of the mothers’ childhood recollections were significantly different. The childhood recollections of the MCDB were significantly more defensive (i.e., Total Defensiveness) than the recollections of the MNB. As compared to the MNB, the childhood recollections of the MCDB (a) were significantly more incoherent; (b) were significantly lacking in specific details (i.e., greater inability to recall); and (c) exhibited a significantly greater amount of unresolve regarding their own childhood experiences. The two groups did not differ in terms of the amount of idealization exhibited in their childhood memories. Table 13 shows the intercorrelations between all of the scales of the AAQ. Most of the Experiential Scales are significantly correlated with each other, as are all of the affecitve-cognitive organizational scales. However, as can be seen in Table 13, there is virtually no support for Hypothesis 10, since there is practically no relationship between problematic childhood experiences and a defensive organization of childhood memories. For 132 Table 13 In r ' n Between Scales f th AA 5.9% l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 ll 1. Unlove 2. Reject .82* 3. Neglect .63" .50" 4. Rol-Rev .33“ .25“ .12 5. Achieve .25" .24" .06 .11 6. Abuse .51" .54" .24" .14* .17* 7. Total‘ .85" .81" .62" .50" .50" .66" 8. Incober .11 .06 .01 -.03 -.04 .05 .03 9. Unresolve .16"I .09 .08 -.01 -.15"' .15“ .05 .71“ 10.NORecall .15* .07 .13 -.05 -.l7* .00 .00 .54“ .51“ 11.1dealize -.05 -.04 -.09 .03 -.03 -.14* -.08 .48“ .33" .25""ll l2.TotDefen" -.05 .06 .04 -.02 -.12 .01 .00 .87" .81" .74“ .69* fl = 133. ‘Total Problematic Relations. "Total Defensiveness. ’o < .05, one-tailed. "o < .01, one-tailed. 133 example, there is literally no relationship between the two composite variables (i.e. , Total Problematic Relations and Total Defensiveness). The three relevant significant relationships are between Unloving and Unresolved, Unloving and Lack of Recall, and Abuse and Unresolved. The other significant relationships (between childhood experience and affective-cognitive organization) found in Table 13 are not supportive of the hypothesis since the a priori predictions were in terms of positive relationships and the findings are inverse. In summation of the results for the three hypotheses in Group II, the mothers did not differ in terms of the quality of their childhood experiences. Contrary to the prediction in Hypothesis 8, the MCDB did not produce childhood memories that were more emotionally problematic than the MNB. On the other hand, Hypothesis 9 is supported as the MCDB did produce childhood recollections that were significantly more defensive and disorganized. Finally, there is little support for Hypothesis 10 which predicted that problematic childhood experiences would be associated with a defensive affective-cognitive organization. r 111' Rel 'onshi BtweenMam Prc 'n hilh Re 11 'n The results for Hypothesis 11 are mixed. While there is some support to establish a link between the mother’s current perceptual Style and the affective- cognitive organization of her own childhood experiences, there are only a few Significant relationships and they are not always in the direction predicted a priori. As can be seen in Table 14, many of the variables have no relationships at all. 134 Table 14 ’ '.-.III 1:0 5' ‘1' - i ' I'v- I- '-'v Or-..I'on If hilthIiI M‘Cfl', WW mm mm MHZ—611 _NoReeail ME IDIDLEE ME NegNeg" -.14* -.06 -.12 -.17* -.16* NegPos° .02 -. 11 -.06 .06 -.03 NegNeutd .09 . 13 -.04 .00 .06 PosPos‘ —.19* -.05 -.10 -.16* -.16* PosNegf .11 .04 .01 .12 .09 PosNeut‘ -.02 .04 -.01 .06 .02 NegProII .08 .05 -.02 .00 .03 CBRSI .18“ .08 .10 .21** .18* CBRSII . 12 .02 -.05 .09 .05 Note. All the correlations are based on M = 133. 'Total Defensiveness. "# of Negative statements correctly identified. ‘# of Positive statements judged to be negative. ‘# of Neutral statements judged to be negative. at of Positive statements correctly identified. ’# of Negative Statements judged to be Positive. g# of Neutral statements judged to be positive. blProportion of negative responses. '9 < .05, one-tailed. "o < .01, one-tailed. 135 Total Defensiveness is significantly inversely related both to the ability to correctly identify Negative statements and to the ability to correctly identify Positive statements. While the hypothesis predicted this inverse relationship between Defensiveness and the number of positive behaviors judged to be positive, it also predicted a positive relationship between Defensiveness and the number of negative statements judged to be negative. Additionally, consistent with the a priori predictions, Defensiveness is significantly related to the mothers’ ratings of behaviors as aversive. It is important to note that both Incoherence and Unresolve have the exact same pattern of relationships to the same three perceptual variables as described above for Total Defensiveness, both in terms of what was originally predicted and the actual results. - l i Upon completion of the analysis reported above, a second statistical analysis was undertaken on a sub-sample selected from the original 133 participants. The rationale for this analysis is twofold; first, this second analysis provided an opportunity to make the group of MCDB more homogeneous in terms of the type of conduct disturbance. Secondly, the sub-sample analysis allowed for a testing of the hypotheses between more "extreme" and/or distinct groups by selectively enhancing the magnitude of the deviancy, as well as the level of "normalcy". The literature reviewed in the introductory chapter clearly states that disturbances of conduct are heterogeneous. There are at least two distinct types of 136 conduct disorders, an Unsocialized Aggressive and a Socialized Aggressive pattern. Researchers have suggested that there are different developmental and family processes which underlie the emergence of overt/confrontive antisocial behavior as compared to covert/concealed antisocial behavior. While the conceptual foundation for the present research is clearly based on assumptions pertaining to what Patterson (1982) referred to as an " overt antisocial pattern" or "Social Aggressors" , the determination of a conduct disturbance for the original 71 participants included in the group of MCDB was based solely on the mothers’ judgement of their sons as having a behavior problem. Hence, the type of conduct disturbance was not determined and/or specified. Even though the literature strongly suggested differentiating between the two types of conduct disturbances, this was not, however, readily possible with the present sample. The data from the CBCL indicated a strong relationship between the Aggression (i.e., overt antisocial) and the Delinquency (i.e. , covert antisocial) scales, I (133) = .69, o < .001. While this significant relationship is based somewhat on overlapping items, it nevertheless suggests that making a theoretical distinction between the types of conduct disturbances may be an easier task than the practical differentiation, as the current data support the notion of a " mixed " type of conduct disturbance (Loeber & Schmaling, 1985a). The formation of a sub-sample did provide an Opportunity to "purify" or refine the original sample in terms of the type of conduct disturbance under investigation. The criteria used for inclusion in this refined conduct-disturbed group was based del CE 5165 relz “it Opp .1- I") r (I!) H ind , 137 primarily on criteria established by Patterson (1982). He differentiated between "Social Aggressors" and "Stealers"; the designation of a "Stealer" was based on the criterion of was "stealing at high rates" as defined by "three or more events" (p.318). In fact, infrequent incidences of stealing were reported for both the "A g gressors" and the "Normals". Thus, for the present sub-sample analysis "stealers" were excluded. The determination of a " stealer" was based on the mother’s response to two items on the CBCL; one item referring to stealing at home and the other item referred to stealing outside the home. On the CBCL, if a mother indicated "frequently" to either of the stealing-related items, or if she checked-off "occasionally" to both of the stealing- related items, then the participant was excluded in the sub-sample analysis. A mother was not excluded on the basis of checking-off "occasionally" to only one of the stealing-related items. The second rationale for the formation of a sub-sample was to provide an opportunity to test the hypotheses under an "extreme case" situation, leaving no doubt about the conduct disturbed status of the target child. Even though the data from the CBCL, especially for the Aggression scale, indicated that the MCDB were significantly different from the MNB, there was, however, considerable overlap in the initial sample between the two groups in their distribution of Aggression scores. For example, there were some MNB who scored in the upper percentiles on Aggression, as well as those MCDB who said that their son was a behavioral problem and yet checked-off practically no items on the Aggression scale. Hence, this sub- II 138 sample analysis provided an opportunity to "clean up " the groups, creating more homogeneity within each group, and thus a greater differentiation or distinction between the groups in terms of their aggression versus non-aggression status (i.e., their conduct-disturbed versus non-conduct disturbed status). The new groups were formed as follows: From the original sample of 133 participants, those mothers with the 30 highest scores on the Aggression scale of the CBCL were included in the group of MCDB. (There were actually 36 mothers since some of the Aggression scores had a frequency of more than one participant.) A few of these mothers were excluded because the target son was determined to be a "stealer" , or because during the initial phone interview the mother had not considered her son to be a behavior problem. There were 28 mothers eventually included in this group of MCDB for the sub-sample analysis. The mothers that making up the group of MNB had the lowest 30 Aggression scores from the original 133 participants. (Once again some of the scores were assigned to more than one participant; thus, the 30 lowest scores included 35 mothers.) Five of these mothers were excluded because during the initial phone interview they had detemiined that their son was a behavior problem at home. There were 30 mothers that were included in this group of MNB for the sub-sample analysis. As expected, there was an extremely large difference in the Aggression scores, as the MCDB M = 24.07, SD = 4.5) were significantly higher than the MNB M = 3.63, SD = 3.1), i (47) = 20.09, p < .001, two-tailed. Additionally, there was 139 a similarly large difference between the groups in terms of the total amount of problems described on the CBCL, as the MCDB M = 63.96, SD = 16.2) described a significantly higher number of problems than the MNB M = 15.37, S_D = 10.8), t(46) = 13.37, p <.001, two-tailed. All of the original hypotheses were re-tested except for Hypothesis 10 which focussed on issues of internal consistency for the AAQ. Table 15 shows the results of Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2. A few of the results are different from the initial analysis. All of the originally significant results were significant for the sub—sample except that the two groups did not differ in terms of their " response tendency". Also, different from the intial analysis, the MCDB rated behaviors as significantly more aversive than did the MNB, and the MCDB also rated behaviors as significantly more desirable than did the MNB. Once again there is no support for the notion of a "behavior classification covariation". Table 16, however, does reveal some interesting relationships between measures from different instruments. For example, the mother’s ratings of aversiveness (i.e., CBRSI) are significantly related: (a) to a "negative response tendency"; (b) to her proclivity to see neutral behavior as negative; and (c) is significantly inversely related to the number of correctly identified positive behaviors. Furthermore, the mother’s ratings of desirability (i.e., CBRSII) are significantly related to her proclivity to judge neutral behavior as positive, and the number of negative behaviors incorrectly judged as positive. These two rating scales (i.e., the CBRSI and CBRSII) are also significantly correlated. 140 Table 15 Comoeo'son of the Deviancy Categorization Process Between MCDB and MNB in the SultSampIIL mm: M DB _N_BM l. The MCDB correctly identified a significantly greater number of negative M 37.89 34.80 behavioral segments than did the MNB. S_D 3.76 6.24 ;(48) = 2.30‘ 2. There was not a significant difference between the MCDB and the MNB in terms of M .46 .36 the number of prosocial behavioral segments SD .63 .67 judged incorrectly as negative. I (56) = .57 n.s. 3. The MCDB judged a significantly greater M 10.89 7.53 number of neutral behavioral segments to be M) 4.89 4.55 deviant than did the MNB. t (56) = 2.71” 4. Overall, the MCDB identified a greater M 50.04 43.40 number of behaviors as negative than did the SD 7.11 9.80 MNB. 1(56) = 2.93" 14 1 Table 15 continued Comparisons 5. Within each mother’s specific response rate, the MCDB did not identify a significantly greater proportion of behaviors as negative than did the MNB. 6. The MCDB did rate behaviors as significantly more aversive than the ratings of the MNB. 7. The number of prosocial behaviors correctly identified by the MNB was not significantly different from those correctly identified by the MCDB. 8. The was no difference between the MCDB and MNB in their tendency to incorrectly identify negative behavioral segments as prosocial. M 39.55 37.63 SQ 7.18 6.20 1(56) = 1.09 n.s. M 141.75 123.26 SQ 21.88 24.29 t (56) = 3.04” M 52.50 51.13 SQ 7.00 11.06 i(49) = .57 n.s. l3 1.11 1.50 Ian D 2.31 2.04 i(56)= .69 n.s. 142 Table 15 continued Comparisons M DB MEI; 9. The MNB did not judge a significantly M 25.25 22.13 greater number of neutral behavioral segments SD 13.17 12.56 to be prosocial. t(56) = .92 n.s. 10. Overall, the MNB did not identify a M 78.89 74.77 significantly greater number of behaviors as SQ 19.85 22.38 positive than did the MCDB. ;(56) = 1.09 n.s. 11. Within each mother’s specific response M 60.45 62.37 rate, the MNB did not identify a significantly SQ 7.18 6.20 greater proportion of behaviors as positive than did the MCDB. 1(56) = 1.09 n.s. 143 Table 15 continued Comparisons M DB m 12. The MNB did not rate behaviors as M 198.50 181.07 significantly more desirable than the ratings of _SQ 25.19 36.27 the MCDB. t(56) = 2.11" While this difference is statistically significant, it is not in the direction predicted. 'o < .05, two-tailed. "o < .01, 144 Table 16 :r I ' III_ B in 'Pr‘rIIrt V 'bls fr 'SUI-. manual 2mm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. NegNeg‘ 2. NegPos" .19 3. NegNeutc .44" .28“ 4. PosPos‘ .58“ .12 .26* 5. PosNeg‘ -.32** -.04 -.07 .16 6. PosNeutf .23“ .15 .33“ .67** .44** 7. NegPro‘ .12 .06 .25* -.66** -.53** -.71** 8. CBRSI“ .09 -.03 .28“ -.3l** -.12 -.15 .45“ 9. CBRSIIi -.17 -.06 .04 -.05 .24* .25“ -.16 .39“ 10.13'TQi -.04 .07 .04 .03 .11 .18 -.15 -.02 11.Responsek .59** .23* .54** .87“ .28“ .88“ -.56** -.12 .11 .10 .11 Note. All the correlations are based on M = 58. ‘# of Negative statements correctly identified. b# of Positive statements judged to be negative. °# of Neutral statements judged to be negative. “I! of Positive statements correctly identified. “19’ of Negative statements judged to be Positive. f# of Neutral statements judged to be positive. ‘Proportion of negative responses. hThe mothers’ ratings of the degree of negativity of negative behaviors. iThe mothers’ ratings of the degree of desirability of prosocial behaviors. JThe Family Togethemess Questionnaire is an index of the family’s enmeshment. kResponse is the total number of behaviors identified on the CBS and is taken as an index of the mothers’ responsivity. ‘o < .05, one-tailed. "o < .01, one-tailed. 145 The MCDB M = 10.5, SQ = 4.0) did describe their families as significantly more enmeshed than the MNB M = 8.3, SQ = 2.5), t (45) = 2.48, o < .05, two- tailed. While the MCDB M = 129, SQ = 21.5) did respond to more behaviors than did the MNB M = 118, SQ = 29.4), this difference was not statistically different, 1 (56) = 1.58, o > .05. Also, as can be seen in Table 16, there is no relationship between responsivity and enmeshment. In fact, the level of enmeshment was not significantly related to any of the perceptual variables. As can be seen in Table 17, the childhood recollections of the MCDB did not exhibit a significantly different amount of problematic emotional experiences in childhood than did the recollections of the MNB. The only exception was that the MNB did experience a significantly greater amount of role-reversal. While the content of these memories did not differ, similar to the initial analysis, the MCDB did produce memories that were significantly more defensive and disorganized than the recollections of the MNB. As compared to the MNB, the MCDB childhood recollections were significantly more incoherent, were more unresolved regarding problematic emotional childhood experiences, and they exhibited a significant lack of specific details. Table 18 has the results of these comparisons. Although frequently not in the direction of the a priori predictions, Table 19 shows that there are some relationships between the affective-cognitive organization of childhood memories and the perceptual variables. For example, the composite score of Total Defensiveness is significantly inversely related to the "accuracy" scores, i.e, correctly identified positive behaviors and correctly identified negative 146 Table 17 II I.«._r-Ir fIr ‘ I- -. ole B 111 ‘ M DB I I ‘ MNB In ‘ Inten 0 Th' ' h R llec'on Contendeales: M DB m t— (510 Unloving M 7.34 7.27 . 12 (56) _SQ 2.00 2.11 Rejection M 6.57 6.64 . 13 (56) SQ 2.08 2.07 Neglect M 4.67 4.56 .18 (56) E 2.21 2.85 Role-Reversal M 4.41 5.5 2.34“ (56) SQ 1.61 1.95 Pressure to Achieve M 4.09 4.52 .71 (56) SQ 2.11 2.52 Abuse M 5 .55 4.75 1.35 (56) SQ 2.27 2.22 Total Problematic Relations M 31.66 32.69 .74 (131) S_D 8.35 7.72 Note. All of the scales were scored in the same direction with higher scores indicating greater problematic experiences. '9 < .05 147 Table 18 II It; I B . I-S Ile ofM DB I. I ‘ MNB on th Aff tiv- ' 'v ' fTh ' hi1 h c 11 55913.5; M DB L“ I: (SID Incoherent M 3 .78 3. 10 2.54” (56) S_Q .91 .97 Unresolve M 3.64 3.10 1.93'" (56) S_Q .96 1.16 Idealized M 2.67 2.49 .56 (56) SQ 1.31 1.15 Lack of Recall M 3.18 2.72 1.67. (56) SQ .96 1.05 Total Defensiveness M 13.28 11.41 2.04" (56) SQ 3.18 3.74 Mote. All of the scales were scored in the same direction with higher scores indicating greater affective-cognitive disorganization or defensiveness. ‘fl = 58. ’o < .05, one-tailed. ”o < .05, two-tailed. 148 Table 19 ’t‘ !°l LI : Aff 'Iv- I'mIV‘ Ort. 'zaII'n If hilIIhIlI Mmoris ..-II J' Def, D‘v. ..;°I.Q..IInP,I; ; '1 - .I- ., .1- Bergem Ineohereni idifl'eeo No Reg! unreeolveo Ioneieri: NegNegb -.17 -.14 -.16 -.27* -.23* NegPos° .07 .00 -.06 .02 .00 NegNeutd .18 .20 .01 .07 . 15 PosPos‘ -.29* -.19 -.35** -.22* -.32** PosNegf .12 .12 -.07 .17 .11 PosNeut‘ . 15 .07 -.02 . 19 . 12 NegProII .09 . 12 .14 -.04 .09 CBRSI .43" .19 .38" .31“ .40" CBRSII .16 .11 .15 .03 .14 Note. All the correlations are based on M = 58. ‘Total Defensiveness. b# of Negative statements correctly identified. °# of Positive statements judged to be negative. “I! of Neutral statements judged to be negative. °# of Positive statements correctly identified. 9:, of Negative statements judged to be Positive. 8# of Neutral statements judged to be positive. "Proportion of negative responses. ’2 < .05, one-tailed. "p < .01, one-tailed. 149 behaviors. Total defensiveness is also significantly correlated with the mothers’ aversiveness ratings. The dimensions of incoherence, inability to recall, and unresolve, all exhibit a similar, but not identical, pattern of results. In summation, the results of the sub-sample analysis are as follows: The MCDB are overly inclusive in their categorization of deviancy, but not exclusive in their categorization of prosocial behaviors. There are no differences between the two groups of mothers in terms of their classification of prosocial behaviors. And, there is no indication that an emphasis in classifying child behaviors as deviant has an inverse relationship with an emphasis on classifying behaviors as prosocial. Different than the initial analysis, the MCDB did, as predicted, rate behaviors as significantly more aversive; and, contrary to the original predictions, the MCDB also rated behaviors as significantly more desirable. Finally, the MCDB describe their families as more enmeshed, but enmeshment has no significant relationship with any of the other perceptual variables. The results of the sub-sample analysis indicate that the two groups of mothers do not exhibit any significant differences in terms of the content of their childhood recollections. The MCDB do, however, produce childhood recollections that are significantly more defensive and disorganized than those produced by the MNB. Finally, although basically not in the direction originally predicted, there are some significant relationships between the mother’s affective-cognitive organization of childhood memories and her perception of child behaviors. The Lack of Detail scale 150 exhibits this significant relationship to the perceptual variables only in the sub-sample analysis. It is important to point out that these results from the sub-sample analysis are basically the same pattern of findings as the results from the initial analysis. The differences between the two analysis are slight and subtle. The main effect of "purifying" the sample was a slight general increase in the effect size (i.e., the magnitude of significant correlations was increased and/or the significant differences between the group means were enhanced). The main exception to this generalization is that in the sub-sample analysis the MCDB gave higher ratings to both deviancy and prosocial behaviors. CHAPTER4 DISCUSSION Throughout this study, the ideas presented and explored have been organized around three main groupings: maternal perception, childhood recollections, and the "linkage" or relationship between perception and the childhood recollections. Within each of these three respective domains, the statistical results are equivocal with a mixture of both supportive and non-supportive findings. For example, while there is not total support for the deviancy categorization hypotheses, there is, however, evidence to support the notion that MCDB are more inclusive in their classification of deviant behaviors and they are more "sensitized" to deviant behaviors. And, while the MCDB do not exhibit differences from the MNB in the content of their childhood memories, the affective-cognitive organization of these recollections does appear to be significantly more defensive/disorganized. Finally, even though the hypothesis predicting a relationship between the mothers’ deviancy classification schema and the affective-cognitive organization of their childhood memories is not supported, there are, however, significant findings that do offer support, albeit suggestively/tentatively, for the underlying notion of linkage, and thus for the construct of an internal mental model. 151 152 Overall, the conceptual framework proposed in the introductory chapter cannot be accepted in its entirety since a substantial number of the hypotheses are not supported. On the other hand, there are a variety of significant findings that do support/confirm the a priori predictions, as well as a number of results that are both interesting and germane to the issues raised in the Introduction. This chapter will reconcile these statistical results with the conceptual model being investigated. Within each group of hypotheses, it will be useful to: (a) examine the pattern of significant and non-significant findings, (b) present the pos hoc analyses undertaken to help clarify some of the results, (c) explore various methodological issues, and (d) raise relevant questions\issues and propose potential directions for further research. Finally, this chapter will conclude with a brief summary and discussion regarding the implications of this research. Throughout this chapter the interpretation of the data will be approached with a fair degree of caution and the findings will generally (but not completely) be regarded as preliminary, suggestive, and encouraging of further research. The rationale for this conservative perspective in the interpretation of the data is as follows: The sheer number of statistical tests performed during the analysis of the data inflates the probability of a Type I error (Myers, 1979). Hence, even though both the actual number of significant findings and the pattern of these significant results (i.e., many of the significant results fit together suggesting a conceptual cohesiveness/coherency) argue against the likelihood of these being spurious findings, a degree of caution or tempered enthusiasm, nevertheless, still needs to be exercised 153 in the interpretation of this data. Additionally, while many of the hypotheses were directional and therefore the use of one-tail tests was appropriate, some of the hypotheses, especially in the large sample analysis, would not have been supported or accepted if a two-tail criterion had been applied. This suggests that the "effect size", or the actual differences between the means is, in some cases, actually quite small. Therefore, patterns of significant results will be emphasized rather than focusing on any one specific significant finding. Finally, much of the methodology employed in this research, while based on previous research, was created specifically for this study (i.e., the instruments were being used for the first time in this study). This lack of a proven track record for many of the instruments also favors a conservative approach towards the interpretation of the data. Mejem Perception Many of the results examined in this section are from hypotheses which were derived directly from Patterson’s (1982,1986b) observations and conceptualizations; specifically, his position that mothers of antisocial children are overly enmeshed and very irritable, and have a perceptual style (i.e. , deviancy-classification schema) which promotes negative interactions (i.e., these parents tend to classify many more child behaviors as deviant and also tend to view many of the child’s acts as worthy of high intensity scolding). As reviewed in the introduction, Patterson hypothesized that this inclusiveness in the classification of deviancy is combined with an exclusiveness in the classification of prosocial behavior; thus further contributing to the coercive/negative and irritable ambience that characterizes these families. 154 It will be helpful, in the following examination of the results, to make a distinction between the mother’s deviancy classification processes, and her ability to track or identify specific child behaviors. These can be conceptualized as two distinct, yet overlapping, cognitive processes. Obviously, it is not always possible to determine the specific influence between tracking skills and the deviancy- classification schema since these cognitive processes can be so highly interrelated. Nevertheless, it seems reasonable to propose that the ability to correctly identify a behavior, either as deviant or prosocial, while reflecting both cognitive processes, be interpreted as primarily an indication of tracking. On the other hand, classifying a neutral behavior as either positive or negative is more likely to reflect an underlying deviancy-classification schema. Additionally, "errors" in labelling (e. g., classifying a deviant behavior as positive) are also very likely to be an indication of one’s classification schema. There is a pattern of findings which clearly indicates that the MCDB have both an overly inclusive cognitive schema for categorizing child behaviors as deviant and are more sensitized and attentive in the tracking of deviant behaviors. (This is one of the areas of significant findings that do not need to be interpreted with excessive caution.) Consistent with previous findings (Lorber, 1981), the MCDB did rate a significantly greater number of behaviors to be deviant. In the initial analysis, these mothers identified a significantly greater proportion of behaviors to be deviant, as compared to the MNB; thus, exhibiting a "negative response tendency". (This proportion score is the percentage of all behaviors identified as deviant calculated for 155 each mother’s specific response rate.) Patterson (1986b) reported that in the Lorber study, the differences were not in terms of accuracy, but rather the mothers of antisocial boys tended to judge behaviors as deviant that were not classified as deviant by trained observers. In the present study, however, the MCDB do demonstrate a significantly greater accuracy in their tracking of deviant behaviors, as well as a significantly greater tendency to also classify neutral child behaviors as deviant. Thus, the greater overall number of behaviors rated as deviant is the result of both tracking and the deviancy-classification schema. The superior ability of the MCDB to identify/recognize deviancy suggest that these mothers are primed and highly attuned to respond to any, and practically all, displays of child misbehavior. For example, in the sub-sample analysis, the MCDB had an average "hit" rate for deviant behaviors of 90% (i.e., correct identification of deviant behaviors) compared to an 82% hit rate for the normal mothers. It’s as if no deviant behaviors escape these mothers’ attention. The interpretation of these findings can be turned around to suggest that normal mothers exhibit an insensitivity to deviancy. The results suggest that normal mothers do not track deviant behaviors with the same vigilance as do the MCDB. As noted in the introductory chapter, Holleran et al. (1982b) reported that lower sensitivity to negative behaviors on the tracking task was associated with higher rates of positive behaviors in the home. They suggested that the normal parents’ "insensitivity" to negative behavior may be in some way responsible for the positive social atmosphere in the home. It was also suggested previously that sometimes the 156 most appropriate response to negative child behavior is no response (Patterson, 1982; Sallow, 1972). The MCDB also exhibit a significantly greater inclusiveness in their classification of deviancy. In both the initial and sub-sample analysis, these mothers classified a significantly greater number of neutral behaviors to be deviant, as compared to the normal mothers. In the sub-sample, the MCDB responded to 14% of the neutral behaviors as if they were deviant, compared to only 8 % for the normal mothers. This finding strongly supports the position that the MCDB have an overly inclusive deviancy-categorization schema. Relatedly, as mentioned in the Introduction, one of Patterson’s (1982) measures of the mother’s irritability is a "crossover". This refers to the mother initiating an aversive interaction (i.e., when she reacts negatively to the child’s prosocial or neutral behavior). The findings regarding the tendency of the MCDB to view neutral behaviors as deviant promotes the perspective that the mother’s irritability, as evidenced by her "crossover", may very likely be, in large part, the result of an underlying labeflmg or categorization process. The MCDB not only perceive a wider range of child behaviors to be deviant and are inclined to be more sensitive/vigilant towards these deviant behaviors, but furthermore, these mothers also appear to be more highly "reactive" to displays of child misbehavior. This final contention is supported by the findings in the sub- sample analysis that the MCDB rated child misbehavior to be significantly more aversive as compared to the ratings of normal mothers. (In the large sample analysis 157 there were no differences between the mothers’ ratings.) This would support Patterson’s observation that these mothers react strongly at the slightest provocation and tend to view trivial instances of child misbehavior as worthy of high intensity scolding. It is reasonable to conceptualize the existence of a behavior-discrimination schema and to speculate that this cognitive system has a direct effect on the mothers’ responsiveness. In other words, there are discriminatory functions involved in the cognitive processes of making a series of differentiations and evaluations that ultimately lead to the mothers’ ratings regarding the severity of the child’s misbehavior. Furthermore, the outcome of these cognitive processes (i.e. , the ratings) is likely to play a critical role in governing a mother’s level of responsivity/reactivity. For example, the affective response to a misbehavior judged to be "extremely serious" is bound to be quite different than the reaction to a behavior judged to be "a slight misbehavior". Thus, the cognitive processes are directly linked with the modulation of the mothers’ affective response. This line of speculation implies the importance of a cognitive-affective dimension that is likely to be involved in determining the overall quality and intensity of a mother’s responsiveness. Clearly, a meaningful pattern of results emerges when examining the five significant findings: The results for the MCDB exhibit (a) a greater number of behaviors identified as deviant, (b) a greater proportion of all identified behaviors identified as deviant behaviors, (c) a greater number of deviant behaviors correctly identified, ((1) a greater number of neutral behaviors classified as deviant, and (e) 158 higher aversiveness ratings. Certainly, this pattern of results is supportive of Patterson’s observations and speculations. In other words, compared to normal mothers, the MCDB perceive a greater number of behaviors to be deviant, and there is a greater likelihood that the MCDB will perceive a neutral and/or relatively benign child behavior as a misbehavior, judge the "misbehavior" to be relatively more serious, and hence initiate an aversive disciplinary interaction with the child. It is in this way that the mothers’ deviancy classification schema (and their behavioral- discrimination schema) combine to increase the probability of an aversive mother- child interaction and thus contribute directly to the aversive and highly irritable atmosphere that characterizes the families of conduct disturbed children. It is reasonable to conclude that the MCDB perceptual style and, relatedly, their response style are very likely implicated in the maintenance (and possibly the development) of the child’s conduct disturbance. (It remains an open question whether the mother’s perceptual style is the cause, consequence, or cause and consequence of the child’s behavioral disturbance.) Even though a very consistent picture has unfolded, it is nevertheless imperative to highlight some aspects of the results that are not necessarily supportive and compatible with this overall portrayal. The most striking feature of the proposed model that has virtually no support in the data is the lack of differences between the two groups of mothers in terms of their perception and categorization of prosocial behaviors. Inherent in the deviancy-categorization hypothesis is the contention that the MCDB inclusiveness in the classification of deviancy is combined with an 159 exclusiveness in the classification of prosocial behaviors. Underlying this perspective is the assumption of a "behavioral classification covariation". While the MCDB exhibit an inclusiveness in classifying deviancy, this "sensitivity" is not at the expense of an insensitivity (or exclusion) towards the classification of prosocial behaviors. There are no differences between the two groups of mothers in terms of the overall number of prosocial behaviors identified, the number of prosocial behaviors accurately identified, or the number of neutral behaviors classified as prosocial. The only significant difference (found only in the sub-sample analysis) was that MCDB rated prosocial behaviors to be significantly more desirable. This result, to be discussed below, is contrary to the proposed model. Clearly, important elements of the deviancy-classification hypothesis are not supported as the MCDB do not exhibit an exclusiveness (or insensitivity) in the classification of prosocial behaviors. Relatedly, the underlying assumption of a behavioral classification covariation is generally not supported. There is virtually no support for the position that an emphasis on deviancy will covary with a de-emphasis in the classification of prosocial behaviors and vice versa. The results reported in the previous chapter (and only minimally modified by the post hoc analysis reported below) exhibited three Significant positive relationships rather than inverse as predicted. Three out of the four correlations performed in the analysis of the behavioral classification covariation hypothesis used variables derived from the CBS. As can be seen in Table 9, all of the perceptual variables derived from the CBS are significantly 160 related to the level of responsivity. The more the mother responded and underlined/identified behaviors on the CBS, the higher her scores were on all of the variables computed. Hence, the correlation table is so conspicuously influenced by response rate that it is not possible to determine if the variables covary due to some underlying dimension other than response rate. In order to make this determination, partial correlations were computed, holding the effects of responsivity constant. The results of this post hoc analysis are presented in Table 20. Even though the results from this post hoc analysis do not affect the outcome of this hypothesis, the partial correlations do, however, help to clarify some of the data. For example, the partial correlations demonstrate that in fact there is no relationship between correctly identifying positive behaviors and correctly identifying negative behaviors. Nor is there a relationship between incorrectly classifying negative behavior as positive and incorrectly classifying positive behaviors as negative. The partial correlations did, however, reveal that the mother’s tendency to classify neutral behaviors as negative has a significant inverse relationship to her tendency to classify neutral behaviors as positive. This being the only significant result of the post hoc analysis, the decision remains not to accept this hypothesis. Finally, if the classification of deviancy does not covary with the classification of prosocial behaviors, then this raises an interesting issue as to whether there are independent classification schemata that may have different functional and/or structural properties? 1 61 Table 20 Peggi’ Qogeieo'on Coefficients Between the PerceoMal Variables of the CBS Wim the fR nivi H1 n Esteem Madame 1 2 3 4 5 6 l. NegNeg‘ 2. NegPos" .07 3. NegNeut‘ .24* .13 4. PosPos‘ -.O9 -.24* -.45** 5. PosNeg‘ -.49** .06 -.19* -.19* 6. PosNeut‘ -.69"'* -.11 -.44** -.36** .30" 7. NegPro‘ .70“ .20"' -.75** -.56** -.32** -.52*"' Note. All the correlations are based on _N_ = 133 ‘# of Negative statements correctly identified. "# of Positive statements judged to be negative. °# of Neutral statements judged to be negative. ‘# of Positive statements correctly identified. °# of Negative statements judged to be Positive. ’# of Neutral statements judged to be positive. gGiven each mother’s individual total response rate, this is the proportion of her negative responses. ’12 < .05, one-tailed. ”o < .01, one-tailed. 162 In the analysis of the behavioral classification covariation hypothesis one of the unexpected findings was a significant relationship between the mother’s ratings of aversiveness and her ratings of desirability. The explanation of this curious finding is open to speculation. The data suggest that a proclivity to be excessive in the interpretation or evaluation of deviancy is related to a proclivity to be excessive in the interpretation or evaluation of prosocial behaviors. Perhaps the positive correlation between these two rating scales indicates the underlying cognitive-affective dimension of a behavioral-discrimination schema (proposed above) involved in formulating evaluations and interpretations. There are other variables in the data that can be thought of as reflecting this cognitive dimension involved in evaluative and interpretive processes. Of particular interest are "over-interpretations" which are defined as the tendency to attribute meaning, significance, clarity, and/or certainty to the perceptual field. For example, the classification of neutral behaviors as either positive or negative can be comprehended as an instance of an over-interpretation since the mother is attributing meaning to a situation which is, at best, ambiguous, and perhaps even meaningless. Even though completely speculative, there are some significant correlations, nonetheless, across different instruments, which lend support, albeit modestly, to this notion of an underlying cognitive dimension involved in evaluative and interpretive processes. For example, Table 9 and Table 16 show that a statistically significant relationship does exist between the tendency to over-interpret neutral behaviors as deviant (i.e., the CBS) with the excessive evaluation of deviancy (i.e., CBRSI). A 163 significant relationship also exists between the over-interpretation of neutral behaviors as positive and the excessive evaluation of desirability (e.g. , CBRSII). It is important to note that the partial correlations Show that interpreting neutral behavior to be deviant is significantly inversely related to interpreting neutral behaviors to be positive. While clearly conjecturable, the data can be understood to suggest that there is both a general tendency to over-interpret, as well as some evidence which points towards systematic interpretive biases, either towards deviancy or prosocial, respectively. There are some trends in the data, including findings already reviewed, to suggest that MCDB have a greater tendency to over-interpret and that there is a systematic bias towards attributing negative meaning to the child’s behavior. For example, in the sub-sample analysis, the MCDB M = 36.14, S_D_ = 14.95) responded to more neutral behaviors (both positively and negatively) than the normal mothers M = 29.67, SQ = 15.04) and this difference approached significance, t (56) = 1.64, o < .053, one-tailed. Furthermore, as mentioned above, the MCDB differed from the MNB in terms of a greater classification of neutral behaviors as deviant and in the sub-sample analysis, these mothers gave significantly higher ratings to both deviant and prosocial behaviors suggesting that their evaluations of child behaviors are more extreme, regardless of the negative or positive value of the behavior. Undoubtedly, this is a Speculative line of reasoning, but one that nonetheless raises some interesting questions for further research. The most obvious question is 164 why do some mothers over and/or misinterpret? It would make for a fascinating study to explore the nature of these attributions in order to establish if there is some systematic bias that is involved in the over-interpretations. For example, is the attribution of specific meaning to a neutral behavior based on the mother’s affective state, therefore indicating that the distortions are situationally determined, or, are they based on her own developmental and/or personality issues? Hence, are some of the over-interpretations "projective" in nature, or are they reactive? Furthermore, is the mother’s responsiveness to an apparently benign child behavior frequently personalized? In other words, are the mother’s negative attributions regarding the meaning of the child’s behavior frequently combined with the attribution of intent (e.g., "he just does that to annoy me")? These issues go beyond a simple construct of a behavioral-classification and/or discrimination schema in an attempt to elaborate upon the conceptualization and expand our understanding of these hypothetical cognitive-affective processes. In order to grasp the complexity of the processes involve, it will be necessary for future research to address issues related to the specific nature, quality, and possible " motivations" of this cognitive-perceptual and interpretive system. The final step in this line of speculation is to propose that these over- interpretations lead to a higher level of reactivity and that the MCDB higher reactivity level as observed by Patterson (1982) (and suggested by the current data) may play a role in determining the mothers’ "inept family management skills" (Patterson, 1982, 1986a,1986c), specifically in terms of discipline. As proposed above, there is a 165 cognitive-affective dimension which largely determines the mother’s reactivity level. The evaluation and interpretation of the child’s behavior is likely to govern and modulate the intensity of the mother’s affective response. Consistent negative attributions regarding the value and severity of the child’s behaviors is likely to lead to consistently higher levels of intensity and reactivity. Over time, the mother’s higher levels of reactivity are likely to lose their effectiveness. Analogously, in listening to loud music there is a sensory adaptation that takes place so that the music no longer sounds loud. If a mother responds with intensity to a relatively mild misbehavior, how does she increase her impact for a serious misbehavior? Clearly, mothers who respond with a generally higher level of intensity to all behaviors, both positive and/or negative, must lose their effectiveness in terms of both discipline and their "parental reinforcement value" (Snyder & Patterson, 1987; Wahler, 1969). In summation, the hypothetical process begins with the mother’s tendency to over- interpret her son’s behavior which then arouses her to a generally higher level of responsivity with an intensified reaction. Over a period of time, the mother’s relatively unmodulated (and/or undiscriminating) responses subsequently lose their effectiveness and hence, this results (perhaps interacting with a history of noncontingent reinforcement (Snyder, 1977; Wahler, 1969)) in the child’s observed nonresponsiveness (Patterson, 1982; Snyder, 1977). Previous research has consistently demonstrated that parents of behaviorally disturbed children are generally more noncontingent in their responses. The current results offer mixed support for this characterization and some of the nonsupportive 166 results are very likely due to a methodological issue involving the CBS. It is the position of this study that the mother’s classification of the child’s behavior determines, in large part, the quality and intensity of her response and thus, the "misclassification" of a behavior might easily result in a noncontingent response. For example, the MCDB tendency to be excessive in their evaluations of the child’s behavior and their greater proclivity to perceive relatively benign or neutral behaviors as deviant are both likely to be cognitive-perceptual factors which play a critical role in determining the mothers’ noncontingent responses; an example of which would be Patterson’s (1982) "crossover ". On the other hand, there are other perceptual "errors" not found in the data which would also logically lead to noncontingent responses. For example, there is no support for the position that MCDB are more likely to provide positive reinforcement for negative behavior (Patterson, 1982; Sallows, 1972/1973; Shaw, 197 1/ 1972; Snyder, 1977) Since there are no differences between the groups of mothers in terms of their tendency to incorrectly identify a deviant behavior as prosocial. Similarly, there are no data congruent with the position that MCDB are more likely to punish prosocial behavior (Patterson, 1982; Shaw, 1971/1972) since the groups did not differ in terms of the incorrect identification of prosocial behavior as deviant. It is very likely that these two measures did not differentiate the two groups of mothers because the CBS did not elicit, as intended, these specific types of perceptual "errors". For example, in the entire sample (i.e., bl = 133) there were virtually no examples of incorrectly identifying negative behaviors as positive (i.e., 167 M = .38; SQ = .72). Improving the instrument so that it has a greater ability to elicit these types of "errors" would be very helpful for future research. Having data for these specific types of perceptual errors would help to explore issues of perceptual bias and distortion and might help to further elucidate the relationships between parental perceptual style and parental response, specifically how noncontingent parental responses may be based on the "erroneous" perception of the child’s behavior. Even though specific recommendations regarding further instrument development are required, overall, the CBS was a valuable research tool, especially in terms of illuminating the MCDB’ negative response tendency. A review of Table 20 does show a variety of expectable relationships among those scales of the CBS and this offers further support for the validity and internal consistency of this instrument. Many of the variables which involve the classification of behaviors as negative are significantly related with each other and are significantly inversely related to those variables measuring the classification of behaviors as positive. For example, the correct identification of a deviant behavior is significantly related to the classification of a neutral behavior as deviant and the overall proportion of behaviors identified as deviant, respectively; while exhibiting significant inverse relationships to both the number of neutral and negative statements identified as prosocial, respectively. This last correlation supports the dimension of a perceptual accuracy for the classification of deviancy and demonstrates that as accuracy (i.e. , correctly identifying deviant behaviors) increases, inaccuracy (i.e., identifying deviant behaviors as 168 positive) decreases. (This logical relationship is not an inherent outcome of the measurement system as it was possible that "accuracy" could demonstrate a different association with "inaccuracy" .) A similar inverse relationship between accuracy and inaccuracy exists in regards to the classification of prosocial behaviors, but there are no statistical relationships which suggest an overall dimension of a behavioral classification accuracy (i.e., the correct identification of deviancy is not related to the correct identification of prosocial behaviors.) Again, this suggest independent classification schematta. Furthermore, although modest, there are, however, a few significant relationships among variables across different instruments, mentioned above, and this provides support for the construct validity of both the CBS and CBRSI and CBRSII. (See Table 9 and Table 16.) For example, the higher the mother’s ratings of deviancy on the CBRSI, the more neutral items she identified as deviant, the greater proportion of behaviors identified as deviant, and in the sub-sample analysis, the less accurate she was in identifying prosocial behaviors. This indicates an overall negative response tendency or proclivity. And, the higher the mothers ratings of desirability on the CBRSII, the more neutral items she identified as prosocial and the more deviant behaviors she incorrectly identified as prosocial. This suggests an overall positive response tendency. While there is much support for the CBS, not all of the intercorrelations, however, are readily explainable. This is particularly true in the pattern of correlations between those variables involving the classification of behaviors as 169 positive. For example, it is not readily apparent why the accurate identification of positive behavior is inversely related to classifying neutral behaviors as positive or identifying deviant behaviors as positive, respectively. It is unclear whether this reflects a measurement issue or some actual underlying dimension of the cognitive schema involved in the classification of prosocial behaviors. Finally, the remaining hypotheses within this group deal with aspects of the model that predicted that the MCDB would described their families as more enmeshed and that this enmeshment is related to various dimensions of the mothers’ perceptual style. The concept of enmeshment denotes a lack of clear individual "boundaries" and less individual privacy/space, with a greater emphasis on interdependence; all together this implies higher levels of interaction and reactivity. This is consistent with Patterson’s (1982) observations that families of conduct disturbed boys are more enmeshed and highly irritable or reactive. While it is true that MCDB do describe their families as significantly closer (i.e., more enmeshed) than normals, this characterization of family life, however, has no apparent relationship to the mothers’ perceptual style. For example, the data provide no support for a relationship between family closeness and the mother’s responsivity level (i.e., response rate on the CBS). Additionally, there is no support for the characterization that enmeshment is related to a negative response style (i.e., irritability). In fact, there are a few extremely modest significant associations suggesting that the closer the family, the more positive the response style. For example, the closer the description of the family: (a) the more behaviors that were 170 classified as prosocial (l: (133) = .15, o < .05,one-tailed; (b) the greater the mother’s tendency to perceive neutral behaviors as positive, and (c) the greater her positive response tendency (r = .16, o < .05, one-tailed). A closer examination of the actual scores on the FT Q indicates that while the MCDB did describe their families as significantly closer than the descriptions of the normal mothers, the actual scores do not justify the characterization of these families as enmeshed. In the sub-sample analysis, where the MCDB exhibited the highest FTQ scores, the group mean score was 10.5. This is a five-item scale and thus the average response to each item was a 2.1. This rating represents the choice of "fairly urine for our family." In other words, the MCDB clearly do not describe their families as " insisting upon family interdependence at the expense of individual autonomy" (Bloom, 1985, p.237) and therefore, it is inaccurate to refer to these families as enmeshed. (If the overall scores had represented the choices of "fairly true" or "very true of our family", then this characterization might have been appropriate.) Furthermore, the actual differences between the two groups on the FT Q is in terms of the MCDB giving scores of "f_aMy untrue" versus the MNB’ scores of "my untrue". It is not clear what this difference means and it seems unlikely that it actually represents a qualitative difference in terms of family functioning. One of the structural properties which characterizes families is that of family boundaries which are rules that define who participates in interactions and the extent of that involvement. This assessment is made along a continuum between "disengaged" at one extreme (indicating inappropriate and rigid boundaries with a 171 lack of interdependence and interactions), through a "normal" range of clear boundaries, and ending at "enmeshment", the other extreme, indicating diffuse boundaries with a heightened sense of belonging at the sacrifice of individual autonomy (Minuchin, 1974). The FT Q does not assess the family’s functioning throughout the entire continuum but rather examines only the extreme end of enmeshment. It is reasonable to conclude that this restricted range of the FTQ consequently did not allow for an adequate test of those hypotheses exploring the potential relationship between family functioning and the mother’s perceptual style. It is incumbent upon future research to assess the quality of the family’s boundaries across the entire continuum. This will allow for a more thorough examination of the following issues: Given the full range of the continuum, are families of conduct disturbed boys more towards the emneshed end of the continuum? Is there a relationship between the family functioning/structure in terms of boundaries and the mother’s perceptual style, especially in regards to her level of responsivity and irritability? M 011 c 'ons Throughout this study there has been an emphasis on the critical role played by parental affective-cognitive factors in determining parent-child interactions. It is assumed that the mother’s perceptual and response style is the product of a mental model regarding parent-child interactions. Essential to this perspective is the position that this parent-child mental model is based largely on the mother’s own childhood 172 experiences. The hypotheses in this section examined the quality and parameters of this mental model by exploring both the content and the organizational properties of the mothers’ own childhood recollections. The results indicate that while the content of the childhood recollections does not differ between the two groups of mothers, the organizational properties, however, do appear to be related to the psychopathology of the child. The MCDB produced childhood recollections that were significantly more disorganized and defensive than those memories of the normal mothers. Furthermore, even though the initial results do not offer support for Main and Goldwyn’s (1985) contention that problematic childhood experiences, specifically rejection, are related to a defensive organization of this information, a post hoc breakdown and re-analysis of the same data does, however, offer supportive evidence consistent with Main’s findings. Finally, it will be useful in this section to include some comments on methodological considerations regarding the use of the AAQ and the accompanying scoring system. In certain aspects, the findings that the content of the childhood memories did not differ between the two groups of mothers are contrary to current common wisdom. The widely held belief that there is a direct relationship between childhood experiences and adult functioning, specifically parental functioning, is not promoted by these results. There is no support for the position that MCDB, as compared to normals, have had more problematic childhood relationships as these mothers do got; appear to be lacking in certain " supportive developmental experiences" (Belsky, 1984). mc res, exp 0111 hit: Ol’cf; 8131: info” 15.1011 ECO”: tents ‘ 173 In both the initial and sub-sample analysis, the only significant finding was that the MNB experienced a significantly greater amount of role-reversal. In the initial analyses each of the content/scale scores (e. g., Unloving, Rejection, Neglect, etc.) were actually combined total scores as the participant’s experiences with her mother was combined with the participant’s experiences with her father. In a post hoc analysis these scores were analyzed separately. Once again the results indicated that there were virtually no differences between the two groups in the quality of the mothers’ childhood relationships with their own mother and with their own father, respectively. The only significant result was that the group of normal mothers experienced a significantly greater amount of "being pushed to achieve" from their own mothers, as compared to the MCDB, 1 (110) = 1.88, o < .05, one-tailed. On the other hand, the data clearly indicate that the MCDB produced childhood recollections that were significantly more defensive and disorganized. Even though some of the specific significant findings exhibited only small mean differences, taken as a whole, however, the pattern of significant results advances a consistent overall picture. This portrayal shows that the flow and accessibility of relationship- related information was significantly more restricted for the MCDB. The childhood recollections of the MCDB demonstrated a relative lack of information accessibility (i.e., Lack of Specific Detail) and appeared to be more disjointed, splintered and contradictory (i..e., Incoherent). Relatedly, their recollections demonstrated a relatively greater lack of resolution, integration, and hence, a lack of separation and individuation regarding childhood experiences and 174 relationships (i.e., Unresolved). It is in this way that the parent-cth mental model appears to be more defensive. In other words, the MCDB parent-child mental model can be characterized by the exclusion and/or restriction of information, by a lack of integration and synthesis, and by the disorganization, splintering, and fragmentation of relationship-relevant information. These results from the AAQ, in terms of both the lack of differences in content combined with the significant differences in organizational properties, strongly suggest that it is not the quality of the childhood experiences per se that determines adult functioning, specifically parental functioning. Rather, it is very likely the affective- cognitive representation of these experiences that is of critical importance. The psychopathology of the child was related to the mother’s affective-cognitive processing, synthesis, integration, and overall representation of her own childhood experiences and not the actual experiences in-and-of-themselves. Apparently, it is in the encoding, processing and resolution/integration, or lack thereof, of the mother’s childhood relationships that is likely to be of critical importance in terms of the psychopathology of the child. This interpretation is consistent with previous research on parental recollections. (It is important to note that previous studies had reported some differences also in the quality/content of childhood experiences.) As reviewed in the Introduction, it is not only the specific content of the childhood memories but also the organizational properties of these memories that are related to current caregiving behaviors (Crowell & Feldman, 1988; Grossman et al., 1988; Main et al., 1985; 112111 r120 corr 111151 Inth for 3 p5}: i1 513?” 11985) filigree ’0 Whit 13301181 311 then hmonm “hen 1 C1hmel Whom. tale ' “’«4'1111n5jJ 175 Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Ricks, 1985). What seems to be a critical factor in the relationship between childhood events and adult functioning is the mother’s ability to "come to terms" with emotionally difficult childhood experiences. It is the degree to which the emotional impact of childhood experiences has been openly understood and integrated into a cohesive, coherent, fluid/flexible and non-contradictory awareness (or a mental model of integrated understanding) that appears to be related to the psychopathology of the child, and assumptively to the parent’s caregiving behaviors. A few of the protocols stood out as remarkable examples of having resolved extremely difficult childhood experiences. Similar to reports by Main and Goldwyn (1985) these protocols were notable for the striking contrast between the extreme degree of family dysfunction and unhappiness in childhood, compared to the extent to which these extremely problematic childhood experiences had been integrated and resolved. In these protocols the emotional turmoil and negative impact was not denied or blocked access, but rather had been openly processed and integrated into an evenly balanced representation of events and their repercussions. These protocols demonstrated a high degree of resolution, understanding, and forgiveness as the written presentations were noticeably clear, detailed and yet concise, and subsequently extremely poignant. These few mothers demonstrated a courage to openly struggle, confront, and to eventually learn from their particularly problematic early childhood relationships. Ultimately, the struggle of these women is a tribute to the potential of the human spirit. 176 A critical question remains unanswered: When childhood experiences are basically similar, what is it that determines why one mother develops a defensively organized mental model, while another mother struggles towards achieving a meaningful resolution? It is assumed that cognitive/mental defensiveness is related to painful childhood experiences and its function is to block access or to prevent these painful affects from being evoked. How is it that some mothers come to terms with very difficult childhood experiences and become exemplary in terms of parenting (Main & Goldwyn, 1985) while other mothers have a defensive mental model forever blocking access to these painful events? Obviously, there is a potentially wide variety of intervening factor/variables, beyond the scope of this study, that could possibly effect this "outcome" into adulthood. Clearly, life experiences and specifically adult relationships have the potential to have a profound impact on one’s relationship-related mental models. Information obtained in this study suggests that involvement in psychotherapy is not necessarily the determining factor as the groups did not differ in their involvement with psychotherapy. It is possible that the timing of difficult childhood events may be a critical factor in determining the emotional impact and subsequently the degree of defensiveness. For example, the effects of a divorce would be different on a 4 year-old than a 12-year-old. Furthermore, having some " healthy " and supportive relationships or meaningful contact with an adult may play a critical role in ameliorating the negative impact of dysfunctional family relationships. Undoubtedly, one can speculate on a wide range of factors and it is likely that there are many 177 relevant aspects which interact with each other. This is clearly an arena that raises many interesting, valuable, and widely relevant issues and questions for future research. It is important to point out that Main and Goldwyn (1985) reported a relationship between apparent rejection in childhood and a defensive orientation towards these childhood memories. Inherent in the conceptual foundation underlying the AAQ and the accompanying scoring system is the establishment of this relationship. It is assumed that the defensive orientation (i.e., the exclusion and/or the fragmenting of information) is functional and is established in order to block certain types of painful and/or anxiety provoking memories from awareness. The establishment of this statistical relationship between problematic experiences and information inaccessibility (i.e., defensiveness) is an integral step in the construct development and validation of the entire conceptual schema, and specifically in regards to instrumentation. The results reported in the previous chapter do not offer support for this critical/pivotal conceptual relationship between problematic childhood experiences and a cognitive/mental defensiveness towards these memories. In a post hoc re-evaluation of this hypothesis, however, specific support was established. As noted above, in the initial analysis the experiential scales were combined scores; the participant’s experiences with her mother were combined with the experiences with her father. For this re-examination it was reasoned that defensive representational processes could potentially be developed to block painful memories/infonnation relevant to 178 experiences with only one specific parent; combining the experiential scores for both mother and father might possibly cancel out the effects or negative impact from one parent and thus obscure valuable information. Hence, in the post hoc analysis of the data relevant to Hypothesis 10, correlations were performed separately between the affective-cognitive organizational scales and the experiences with mother, and the experiences with father, respectively. AS can be seen in Table 21, four out of the five affective-cognitive organizational scales are significantly correlated with experiencing the mother as Unloving, and three out of the five scales are significantly correlated with the mother’s apparent Rejection. (Table 22 reveals that these relationships do not exist in terms of the experiences with the father.) While these significant relationships are not large statistical associations, nonetheless, the results that emerge from this re- evaluation of the data are strikingly consistent with those results reported by Main and Goldwyn (1985) on the basis of their in—depth interview. The lack of a similar relationship between the experiential scales and the Idealization scale does not conform to previous results. It is very likely that the marginal inter-rater reliability for this scale weakened its usefulness (i.e., the scales’ power to differentiate groups.) The results indicate that the defensiveness in the mother’s recollections, or mental model, is related, to some extent, to problematic experiences with her own mother and not necessarily in terms of the relationship with her father. Given that 35 years ago the typical family structure was characterized by the mother as the primary caregiver, it is reasonable to suggest that mothers were generally more [2' ’To It 179 Table 21 II; ILA-I113 '; ‘ hilIIhOd Exr-rince with the MI er - I Aff 'v- "v ' 'n es ftheAA Seeiee l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1.Unlove 2. Reject .82** 3. Neglect .55** .46** 4. Roi-Rev .37** .19* .12 5. Achieve .24” .20“ .00 .23“ 6. Abuse .47" .48** .22** .10 .18“ 7. Total‘ .88" .80“ .59** .54“ .47** .63“ 8.1ncoher .14* .12 -.03 -.05 -.13 .05 .02 9. Unresolve .22** 20*“ .05 -.03 -.17* .17* .12 .71** 10.NoRecall .23** .22** .12 -.05 -.16* .07 .ll .54** .51** ll.Idealize -.07 .00 -.13 -.03 -.10 —.16* —.13 .48“ .33“ .25** l2.TotDefen" .16* .17* .oo -.05 -.l8* .04 .03 .87" .81** .74** .69** ii = 133. ‘Total Problematic Relations Between the Participant and her Mother. l"Total Defensiveness for the entire AAQ Protocol. ’o < .05, one-tailed. "p < .01, one-tailed. 4. Roi 5. Ach 6. Abu 180 Table 22 In rr ' B n hildhood Ex rience with the F r n 'v- "v r ' 'n sof eAA Scales 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 ll 1. Unlove 2. Reject .80“ 3. Neglect .62** .44** 4. Rol-Rev .11 .14* -.02 5. Achieve .35“ .37** .13 -.01 6. Abuse .54“ .59“ .14* .08 .29" 7. Total‘ .88“ .86** .59” .30” .55** .71** 8.1ncoher .06 -.01 .05 .02 .05 .04 .05 9. Unresolve .06 -.06 .09 .02 -.10 .06 .02 .71** 10.NoRecall .04 -.09 .11 -.03 -.16* -.06 -.05 .54“ .51" ll.Idealize -.03 -.06 -.04 .08 .04 -.08 -.02 .48** .33** .25** 12.TotDcfen" .04 -.08 .07 .02 -.05 -.01 .00 .87** .81** .74** .69** N = 133. ‘Total Problematic Relations Between the Participant and her Father. t’Total Defensiveness for the entire AAQ Protocol. 'o < .05, one-tailed. ”p < .01, one-tailed. influ mod. child 101111 achi 1101181 andt cohes repre. 181 influential and thus had a greater impact on the structure of the participant’s mental model. Furthermore, it was the mother’s unavailability and her turning-away of the child’s attention, affection, and needs that appear to be the critical experiences related to the development of a defensively oriented mental model. It is understandable that a child’s repeated experiencing of her mother as generally unavailable, emotionally nonsupportive, and rejecting could cause a sufficiently intense level of emotional pain and distress so as to initiate (as a means of self-protection in order to maintain cohesiveness) the development of a defensive and distorting affective-cognitive representational style of these events. Overall, it is clear that the AAQ and the accompanying scoring system has been an effective research instrument. This study is an initial attempt to utilize a questionnaire format of the Adult Attachment Interview, with subsequent modifications to the scoring system. The fact that there is support for some of the a priori predictions in terms of group differences and that some meaningful significant relationships, albeit modest, were obtained between the scales is a promising endorsement regarding the potential usefulness of the methodology. In other words, the data offer some important support for the validation of both the instrument (in terms of internal conceptual consistency) and the overall conceptual schema. Even though the magnitude of the associations id not as large as those reported in Main and Goldwyn’s (1985) preliminary report based on the in-depth interview, the similarities in the pattern of results suggest that the questionnaire format may be an appropriate alternative methodology. using exper few 1 isimr inlhe Weigh: disadv. 501116 Ittolle. 311 ovel Emmi Partitip 10 63131] the data 31.08am 182 As compared to the in-depth interview, there are some definite advantages to using the questionnaire format. For example, there is significantly less time and expense involved in the data collection process. The data can be collected within a few hours in a group setting using a standardized format (i.e., there is no need to train and hence establish inter-interviewer reliability). Once the data are collected it is immediately ready to be scored thereby eliminating the time and expense involved in the transcription of the verbal interview. Obviously, the specific needs of the research require that these advantages be weighed against the disadvantages of the written protocols. The prominent disadvantage is that the questionnaire format does not allow the researcher to "probe" , and establish needed clarification of the participants recollections. For example, in some of the written protocols there were many gaps and disparities in the recollections and it was solely up to the individual rater to attempt to piece together an overall picture. Finally, the research on the Adult Attachment Interview has not emphasized issues of scoring at the scale level since the ultimate goal is to assign the participants an attachment-classification. Clearly, future research is needed in order to establish whether this attachment classification can be determined on the basis of the data from the AAQ. The scoring system, which was adapted from Main and Goldwyn ( 1989) appears to have some applicability and usefulness. It is important to point out, however, that neither the researcher nor the raters had any direct training in the original scoring system and furthermore, modifications were made in order to 183 accommodate the written format. Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that there may be many substantive differences between the Main and Goldwyn scoring system and the system that was ultimately used for this study. Additionally, the three scales created for this study, namely, Abuse, Lack of Specificity/Recall, and Unresolved/Autonomy all had good inter-rater reliability. In fact, the Abuse scale had the highest reliability among all ten scales, and the two "new" affective-cognitive scales proved to be useful research tools. It is interesting to note that all of the Experiential scales are significantly intercorrelated. One possible interpretation of this inter—relatedness among the scales is that the scoring system did not sufficiently discriminate between different dimensions or aspects of problematic experiences and that in fact all the scales are measuring basically the same underlying experience. On the other hand, an alternative explanation is that there is a wide range of problematic childhood experiences and that these experiences do in fact overlap and co-exist. For example, it makes sense that Unloving will be highly correlated with all the remaining Experiential scales since the other scales are intended to assess some of the various ways in which a parent might be unloving. It is also quite possible that a parent can be unloving, rejecting, abusive, and role-reversing. Relatedly, it is consistent that Neglect (taken as an index of the lack of emotional/nurturing interactions) is not related to either Role-Reversal or Pressure to Achieve since both of these scales are indices of over-involvement. Hence, it is reasonable to interpret these 184 intercorrelations as suggesting that when the caregiving is lacking and inadequate, this creates a wide variety of concomitant problematic emotional experiences for the child. Finally, even though the inter-rater reliabilities established for the present study were adequate, it might be more effective to score these protocols within a group format. This suggestion originates from the group experiences in the raters training where the final decisions for scoring the protocols were derived through a group process. Each individual rater would come to the training session with two protocols scored independently. When scores were divergent the group process would eventually arrive at a general consensus. The rationale for proposing this type of scoring procedure is as follows: It cannot be overly emphasized that the data from the AAQ is extremely sensitive, intimate, poignant, and emotionally powerful. The participating mothers took the task seriously and became very involved in the process of recollecting. Most of the mothers wrote for over two hours, some cried periodically, and practically all of them were deeply absorbed in their own private experience and introspection. Many mothers reported that the overall experience was extremely worthwhile and they were able to gain valuable insights through the process of completing the questionnaire. Many mothers reported that they spontaneously saw meaningful connections between their own childhoods and their current adult functioning and relationships, especially gaining relevant understandings regarding their current parenting style. Ultimately, these mothers shared very personal and emotionally vulnerable aspects of their childhood and of themselves. (It would be interesting to compare the level of self- 185 disclosure between the "privacy" and anonynrity of the questionnaire format with the level of openness exhibited in the interview data which involves disclosure in the presence of another person.) Inevitably, these private and heart-rending accounts have a strong and noticeable impact on the reader/rater. The recollections are stirring and deeply moving and consequently the reader is highly susceptible to their own personal bias and interpretive distortions. Even though all the raters had previous clinical training, nevertheless, they all unavoidably exhibited some " transference " type distortion/bias in their interpretation of the protocols. The training procedure inadvertently demonstrated the potential moderating effect of a group process towards the individual biases evoked in the scoring of this data. Personal sources of bias were counter- balanced by the group and within this context they became useful sensitivities providing valuable sources of input. Consequently, having group-derived scores for the protocols is a way to minimize and constructively utilize the sources of individual bias and distortion that are inherent in the interpretation of this material, thereby assigning consistently more "objective" and accurate ratings to the data. M R 11 'nandPrce a1 1 The major thrust of this study is to demonstrate a link between a mother’s childhood experiences and her current adult functioning. Specifically, the purpose is to establish a connection between the manner in which a mother has internally organized/ represented her own childhood experiences and her current perceptual style. Conceptrrally, this connection is maintained through the construct of an internal .1. 'y 1 My), 186 mental model regarding parent-cth interactions. It is hypothesized that this parent- child mental model, based largely on the mother’s own childhood experiences, plays a crucial role in determining the mother’s behavioral classification schema. Hence, Hypothesis 11 is an attempt to establish support for the overall notion of linkage and is specifically designed to explore the relationship between a mother’s deviancy categorization processes (i.e. , her current perceptual style) and the affective-cognitive organization of her childhood recollections (i.e., her mental model of parent-child relationships). Even though the Specific predictions involved in Hypothesis 11 are not supported, there are some significant findings that do promote, albeit tentatively, the notion of linkage. The a priori predictions were based on a very specific line of reasoning. These specific pathways that were predicted in order to demonstrate the connection between the mother’s past and her current perceptual style are not found in the data. Nevertheless, a pattern of significant associations does emerge and these results are not inconsistent with the overall conceptualization suggesting that the connection between the mother’s own childhood experiences and her current parenting behavior, specifically her perceptual style, is maintained, in part, via the construct of an internal parent-child mental model. For example, the specific proposition that the mothers’ affective-cognitive defensiveness would be associated with a negative perceptual style was not supported. In other words, it is not t__e that the more disorganized and defensive the mother’s childhood recollections, the greater her tendency to exhibit a categorization schema 187 that is overly inclusive of deviant behaviors and overly exclusive of prosocial behaviors. Interestingly, there is, as predicted, a significant inverse relationship between the mother’s affective-cognitive defensiveness and her correct identification of prosocial behaviors. Clearly, this could be interpreted as part of a negative response style suggesting that the mothers’ tendency to distort, restrict, splinter, and/or disorganize attachment-related information is in fact (as predicted) related to a corresponding need to shift attention away from prosocial behaviors. But this interpretation, which proposes a " selective inattentiveness" towards prosocial behaviors, is likely to be erroneous since the exact same pattern of results is exhibited in regards to the identification of deviant behaviors. Therefore, the results do not favor an interpretation of attentional "selectivity" (or nonresponsiveness to prosocial behaviors) but rather, the data suggest that the defensiveness in the mother’s parent- child mental model appears to be associated with a non-specific overall cognitive- perceptual ineffectiveness or inefficiency regarding the ability to correctly identify child behaviors, both deviant and prosocial. It is incumbent upon this discussion to offer a plausible explanation as to why the predicted associations were not found in the data, and at the same time, to provide a conceptual framework in order to understand the significant associations which are evidenced. Clearly, a wide variety of assumptions were involved in the derivation and ultimate rationale for the a priori predictions. It appears that some of these assumptions are suspect. A re-examination of these underlying postulates will help to provide an explanation regarding the lack of support for the a priori perspective 188 and relatedly, help to establish a slightly modified or alternative understanding/perspective in order to account for the pattern of significant associations that are present. In order to examine some of these questionable assumptions, it will be helpful to briefly review the rationale for the specific predictions involved in Hypothesis 11: The literature emphasized how the MCDB are characterized by their irritability, criticalness, and negativity. This negativity was believed to be exhibited, in part, via a negative perceptual style (i.e., the mothers’ deviancy-classification schema) and hence, it was predicted that the MCDB would be overly inclusive in their classification of deviancy and underinclusive in their classification of prosocial behaviors. It was also reasoned that this hypothesized inclusiveness towards classifying deviancy was the result of an insensitivity, and specifically an inattentiveness, towards prosocial behaviors. This reasoning is based on the pivotal assumption that the negativity which characterizes the MCDB is a parallel process to the rejection and nonresponsiveness which characterizes the mothers of avoidant infants. In other words, this hypothesized nonresponsiveness to prosocial behaviors was seen as reflecting the mothers’ need to shift attention away from attachment- related information (i.e., their defensively organized parent-chfld mental model) as prosocial behaviors were assumed to be more affiliative in nature and hence more relationship oriented. Thus, rejection and nonresponsiveness at a behavioral level was assumed to correspond to a blocking of information and subsequent inattentiveness at the representational level. It was reasoned that turning away from attachment-related 189 information (i.e. , the mental/cognitive defensiveness) resulted in a nonresponsiveness and inattentiveness towards prosocial behaviors and subsequently an over-emphasis on deviancy. Clearly, one of the most obvious underlying assumptions that proved to be erroneous is the "behavioral classification covariation hypothesis". Inherent in the formulation of the specific predictions is the assumption of a classification covariation. But the data (reviewed previously in this chapter) clearly do not support this underlying assumption. Therefore, it was not possible to obtain significant correlations between the defensiveness in the mothers’ mental model and an inclusive- exclusive classification schema, since this pattern of covariation in the classification of child behaviors does not exist in the data. Perhaps the most influential assumption involved in the formulation of the a priori predictions is that "the negativity that characterizes the MCDB is a parallel process to the rejection and nonresponsiveness that characterizes the mothers of avoidant infants". A corollary of this pivotal assumption is that the MCDB adult attachment-classification is likely to be that of "Dismissing of Attachment" (Main & Goldwyn, 1989) and hence, by implication their sons would correspondingly be characterized by an "insecure-avoidant attachment". This set of related assumptions, which upon re-examination are questionable, had a far-reaching impact in the formulation and specific direction of the a priori predictions. Concomitant with these assumptions is the perspective that the MCDB affective-cognitive defensiveness has a Specific purpose or function. In other words, 190 the specific attachment-classification of Disnrissing implies a distinct strategy in the organization and defensiveness of the mental model. Hence, while it is true that adults classified as "Pre-occupied by/Entangled in Past Attachments" (Main & Goldwyn, 1989) also have a defensively organized parent-child mental model that is designed to "block access to attachment-relevant information", the overall function or defensive strategy of this particular affective-cognitive organization is different from that of Dismissing adults. The main point is that the specific organization (or predominant orientation) of the particular parent-cth mental model leads to an explicit set of predictions regarding the mothers’ behavioral classification schema. Predicting a lack of responsiveness to affiliative behaviors, with a subsequent over-emphasis on deviancy, derives directly from the assumption that the mental model of these mothers has a Dismissing orientation. It is this assumption, however, that may have been in error. Clearly, this would account for the lack of confirming results. Furthermore, the actual findings are in fact more congruent with what might be expected if the MCDB attachment-classification was that of Preoccupied. In order to advance this line of reasoning it will be helpful to clarify the distinctions that differentially characterize the affective-cognitive organizations related to these two types of insecure attachments. Main and Goldwyn (1989) do point out that while there are some similarities in the attitudes of insecure adults, nonetheless, "the strategy the adult uses in her thinking about attachments and about relationships is usually predominantly either Disnrissing or Preoccupied" (p.103). 191 "The Dismissing adult generally attempts to ignore, avoid, or devalue attachment relationships and experiences" (Main & Goldwyn, 1989, p.102). The strategy is to de-activate and detach from the emotional impact of relationships which, for the child, were frequently characterized by being unloved and rejected. The defensive deactivation of these events is maintained via their mental model with a primary emphasis on the lack of recall (i.e. , inaccessibility to the potentially painful events) and/or high idealization (i.e. , self-deception regarding the impact of the events). The distinguishing features of their current parenting behaviors are likely to emphasize a rejection, a nonresponsiveness, and an emotional distancing; correspondingly, their own children are likely to exhibit a distancing, rejection and avoidance of close contact. The Pre-occupied adult, on the other hand, "is unable to free herself from either her active concern with her relationships, her traumatic past, or (exhibits an) inability to move beyond her early relationships" (Main & Goldwyn, l989,p.103). While these mothers appear to be "objective", analytical, and very aware of the impact of childhood experiences, their pre-occupation, in fact, reveals a weakened sense of self, separateness, and resolve. This restricted sense of self is probably a way to minimize awareness of the negative emotional impact. The emotionality, when present, seems to lack integration as if it is "unmetabolized" (Main & Goldwyn, 1989). These adults frequently over-intellectualize in an ineffective manner, and with their weak sense of separateness they remain chronically engaged/preoccupied, never able to work-through and establish a boundary or finality to these negative "311.3,? . 192 experiences. The Preoccupied adult characteristically experienced a lack of support as a child, not in terms of rejection, but rather they frequently came from families that were extremely role-reversing and involving/enmeshing (Main & Goldwyn, 1989). The defensive strategy of their mental model is highlighted by a marked incoherence (i.e., the fragmenting and splintering of attachment-relevant information in order to minimize the emotional awareness/impact) and a preoccupation, lack of resolution, and lack of sense of self. All of these qualities aid in denying the irreversibility and finality of these past events. The hallmark of these mothers’ current parenting behavior is their inconsistency, ambivalence, and high reactivity. Correspondingly, their own children are likely to exhibit angry resistant behavior that is mixed with seeking contact and closeness (i.e., reflecting a fundamental ambivalence regarding attachment.) The pivotal (and very likely dubious) assumption that the MCDB are likely to be classified as Dismissing was based on the characterization of these families as rejecting and negative. However, the conduct-disturbed families are also regularly described as inconsistent, enmeshed, and highly irritable and reactive. These additional descriptors do not fit the characterization of an emotionally distant and detached family atmosphere as one would expect with that of Dismissing mothers, but rather they are very congruent with the characterization of Preoccupied mothers. In other words, the inconsistency, high reactivity, enmeshment, "emotionality" and intensity described in the literature on conduct-disturbed families, and generally 193 supported with the present findings, have a striking similarity with the characterization of the Preoccupied adult. Furthermore, it is reasonable to propose that the child’s conduct disturbance appears to exhibit behaviors of an insecure resistant/ambivalent attachment, rather than an insecure/avoidant attachment. This would correspond with the mother’s classification of Preoccupied rather than Dismissing. Greenberg and Spletz (1988, p.206) point out that conduct problems "can be viewed as strategies for gaining the attention or proximity of caregivers who are unresponsive to the child’s other signals." But these inappropriate behaviors also seem to reflect an underlying ambivalence; while the behaviors may be an attempt to seek proximity, they also seem to express an angry resistance and emotional distancing. The enactment of the annoying (i.e., distancing) behaviors exhibited by Social Aggressors, the specific conduct disturbance under investigation (e.g. , teasing, excessive talking, showing off, stubbom, argues, brags, demands attention, etc) clearly necessitates close and frequent interactions with another person, specifically the caregiver. This is contrasted with Delinquency which is much more likely to reflect an insecure-avoidant attachment, as this type of behavioral disturbance involves a noticeable detachment or lack of interaction with the caregiver; for example, Delinquency is characterized in the literature by the lack of parental supervision. On the basis of this line of reasoning, it is reasonable to conclude that the MCDB do not, as originally assumed in the formulation of the a priori predictions, have a parent-child mental model that is primarily Dismissing of attachment. Rather, 194 it is likely that these mothers would be primarily classified as Preoccupied. Obviously, only further research can ultimately resolve this and other related issues. Apparently, one of the shortcomings of this study is its sole reliance on scale scores to describe the mother’s internal parent-child mental model, rather than determining the mother’s overall adult attachment-classification. Main and Goldwyn’s (1989) higher-order variables (i.e., the attachment-classification) do seem to provide an added dimension and depth of understanding to the data not possible from only the individual scale scores. The attachment-classification provides a meaningful way of organizing the different scale scores into a coherent whole. This typology provides a useful conceptual framework and allows for differential predictions/hypotheses in terms of further research. For example, Is the parent’s adult attachment-classification related to the child’s behavioral disturbance? Do the different insecure attachment- classifications relate to different types of "pathology" or behavioral disturbances in the children? Are Social Aggressors predominantly from families with parents classified as Pre-occupied, while the StealerS/Delinquent have parents that are primarily classified as Dismissing? Is their an interaction between adult attachment- classification (including the classification of Secure/Free-Autonomous) and the parents’ current behavioral classification schema? The conjecture that the MCDB may in fact have a Preoccupied attachment- classification would (as stated above) undoubtedly lead to a different set of expectations regarding the pattern or pathway of linkage between the mother’s parent- child mental model and her current perceptual style. The affective-cognitive 195 organization of a primarily Preoccupied parent-child mental model would be reflected in an inconsistent and highly reactive perceptual style. In other words, rather than expecting a nonresponsiveness to prosocial behaviors, as originally predicted, it is likely that the defensive strategy of Preoccupied mothers would result in an erratic, confused, and ineffective perceptual style. It is possible to interpret the significant relationships exhibited in Table 14, and Table 19 for the sub-sample analysis, as consistent with this perspective. For example, overall, affective-cognitive defensiveness (as measured through the four different affective-cognitive scales and the composite score) is significantly inversely related to both the number of correctly identified deviant behaviors and the number of correctly identified prosocial behaviors; as the defensiveness increases, the mother’s ability to accurately perceive both deviant and prosocial behaviors slightly decreases. Also, defensiveness is significantly related to higher aversiveness ratings of child misbehavior. Overall, this pattern of significant findings does indicate that the more the mother’s childhood recollections were disorganized, splintered\fragmented, lacking in detail, and unresolved in terms of childhood relationships, the less the mother’s accuracy in correctly identifying the child’s behaviors, and the more likely she is to be highly reactive (i.e., higher ratings) to deviant behaviors. It is important to point out that the magnitude of the Significant correlations only ranges between very modest to moderate and the observed pattern of significant associations does not exist for all of the defensiveness scales. For example, 196 Idealization does not exhibit any associations with the perceptual variables and the Lack of Detail/Recall scale only exhibits significant relationships in the sub-sample analysis. (Interestingly, the Unresolve scale is conceptually synonymous with the status of Preoccupied and this scale does exhibit the strongest or most impressive associations with the three perceptual variables; this implies that the greater the preoccupation the less accurate the mother’s perceptual style and the more intense her reaction to child misbehavior.) Overall, assuming that the MCDB are in fact Preoccupied adults, the general pattern of associations are consistent with expectations. The defensive affecitve- cognitive organization appears to have resulted in a slight decrease in cognitive- perceptual efficiency and effectiveness and is likely to have also interfered with affect- modulation. A cornerstone of this study is the logical supposition that the mother’s accurate identification, evaluation, and labelling of the child’s behaviors are prerequisites for an appropriate parental response. It is reasonable to propose that an overall reduction in the capacity to correctly identify ’child behaviors combined with a less modulated affective response may be factors involved in the MCDB’ inconsistent, more highly emotional, and noncontingent response style. As discussed below, there are obviously other competing intervening factors which also affect the outcome of the mother’s perceptual style and subsequently, the ultimate quality of her responses. Nevertheless, it is reasonable to conclude that the data do, albeit tentatively, suggest a linkage or connection, maintained via the parent-child mental 5A 197 model, between the mother’s childhood experiences and the quality and appropriateness of her responses to her son’s behavior. The significant positive correlations, especially in the sub-sample analysis, between the mothers’ ratings of child misbehavior (i.e., CBRSI) and the various defensiveness scales offer the most consistent pattern of results promoting the overall conceptualization. There are four conditions that are necessary in order to confidently support the conceptual model. Namely, there needs to be: (a) a significant difference between the groups in terms of the level of cognitive/mental defensiveness; (b) a significant difference between the groups on specific perceptual variables; (c) significant correlations between defensiveness and the perceptual variable, and (d) the direction of the correlations needs to be logically congruent with the scores on the perceptual variable. The relationship between the mothers’ affective-cognitive defensiveness and their level of reactivity to deviancy satisfies all four of the stated criteria. The MCDB are both significantly more defensive and more reactive to deviancy. Therefore, the moderate positive statistical associations between the various scales of defensiveness and the mother’s reactivity to deviancy strongly implicate the MCDB defensively organized parent-child mental model. This pattern of results suggests that the affective-cognitive defensiveness may account, at least in part, for the MCDB higher levels of reactivity and irritability. It was previously proposed that higher reactivity may reflect a behavioral-discrimination schema that has a direct effect on the quality of the mother’s responsiveness (i.e., a cognitive-affective dimension). 198 Therefore, the positive relationship between reactivity to deviancy and cognitive/mental defensiveness suggest that the mother’s parent-child mental model may play a role in the regulation and modulation of the mother’s affective response/reaction. It was already suggested that habitually higher levels of parental reactivity may contribute to an ineffective discipline style. Hence, the data can be interpreted as consistent with the overall conceptualization that the manner in which a mother has mentally organized her own childhood experiences may ultimately affect certain aspects of her current parenting behaviors. On the other hand, a more qualified interpretation is evoked in order to explain the relationships between cognitive/mental defensiveness and the decreased capacity to identify the child’s behavior. Only three out of the four criteria required to firmly support the overall conceptualization are fulfilled. The lack of support comes primarily from the inverse association between defensiveness and the mothers’ ability to correctly identify deviant behavior. One of the goals of this study was to demonstrate that the organizational properties of the mothers’ parent-child mental model would "o ex a crucial role" in detemrining their behavioral-classification schema. This objective was not realized. Even though there is a connection between the organizational properties of the mother’s memories and specific perceptual variables, the pattern of the data definitely indicates that there are other significant factors which may be more substantially involved in determining the mother’s overall classification schema, especially in terms of the MCDB sensitivity to deviancy. 199 For example, it has been clearly demonstrated that the MCDB exhibit a heightened sensitivity to deviant behaviors and they also exhibit childhood memories that are significantly more defensive. Contrary to the goals of this study, however, this heightened awareness towards identifying deviancy is not accounted for by the organizational properties of the mothers’ childhood memories since the data indicate that as defensiveness increases the ability to perceive deviancy decreases. This is the fourth criteria listed above and the inverse relationship between defensiveness and the perceptual variable clearly does not fulfill this condition (i.e., the inverse direction of the correlation is not logically congruent with the group differences in identifying deviant behaviors.) Obviously, there must be some unexplained factor(s) and/or interaction which accounts for the MCDB sensitivity to deviancy. In other words, even though the data suggest a link between the way in which a mother organizes her childhood recollections and specific aspects of her current perceptual style, the results do not ultimately help to explain the MCDB vigilance or heightened awareness to deviant behaviors. This raises issues as to the complexity of interacting forces that are involved in deriving the final outcome/product of a behavioral-classification schema. A better understanding of the nature of these other factors (e.g., perhaps the history of the child’s chronic misbehavior results in a response set and/or the mother’s emotional state due to her marital relationship or the quality of social support systems) might help to explain the MCDB vigilance towards deviancy. This is definitely an area that necessitates further research. 200 In summation, the very nature of the connection between the Specific defensive affective-cognitive properties of the mother’s childhood recollections and her current functioning remain largely, but not completely, elusive and unexplained. Clearly, the specific and detailed pathways of this connection and the actual amount of the variance that can be accounted for, are critical issues in need of replicated findings and further research. It is clear that other (and as of yet unexplained) factors, besides the organization of childhood memories, also have a strong impact on the mother’s behavioral-classification schema and ultimately on her response style. On the other hand, the statistical associations, albeit modest, between the affective-cognitive organization of the mother’s recollections and specific aspects of her perceptual style are evidenced. The way in which a mother identifies, evaluates, and classifies child behaviors can be explained or accounted for, to a modest degree, by an understanding of the way in which she has organized her own childhood experiences. Undoubtedly, there is a link between the way in which a mother organizes her own childhood memories and her current functioning, specifically her cognitive-affective functioning and certain aspects of her perceptual style. The results of this study do encourage continued research designed to further explore and elucidate the construct of an internal parent-child mental model which serves as a mediator between past childhood events and current adult functioning. W The goal of this study was to demonstrate a link or connection between a mother’s own childhood experiences and her current parenting style, specifically her 201 perceptual style. This connection between past events and current functioning is maintained via the construct of a parent-child mental model which is based on the mother’s own childhood experiences. In other words, the internal representation (and specifically the affective-cognitive organization) of the mother’s own childhood experiences is believed to guide and govern the quality of her current responses to her children. While a child’s conduct-disturbance is a legitimate research topic in its own right, the family and relationship dynamics hypothesized by Patterson (1982), and believed to be at the core of the child’s behavioral disturbance, do provide a useful paradigm for examining the relationship between the mother’s own childhood experiences and her current parenting behaviors. This is primarily due to the importance ascribed to the mother’s cognitive processes (i.e. , her behavioral classification schema) in the development and maintenance of the child’s conduct disturbance. The ways in which a mother perceives her son’s behavior undoubtedly affects how she interacts with him, and consequently, how the child behaves. This classification of the child’s behavior involves hypothetical cognitive processes and it is here that the connection (via the mother’s internal parent-child mental model) between past and present is established. The organizational properties of the mother’s parent—child mental model reflect how relevant information is processed, accessed, and integrated; a defensive organization refers to the tendency to distort, restrict and/or inhibit certain classes of information. It is this representational model that is 202 believed to play a critical role in determining the mother’s current cognitive- perceptual behaviors. In order to support this overall conceptualization it was necessary to (a) establish differences between the two groups of mothers in regards to their perceptual style (i.e., behavioral classification schema); (b) to establish differences between the groups in regards to the content and organizational properties of the mother’s childhood recollections (i.e., the parent-cth mental model); and (c) to establish a meaningful connection between the mother’s perceptual style and her mental model. Even though there were supportive findings partially satisfying each of these criterion, overall, however, the results were equivocal. For example, the data indicated that the MCDB are more sensitive and inclusive in their classification of deviancy and they also exhibited childhood recollections that were more disjointed, fragmented, lacking in specific detail, and were generally more unresolved regarding childhood relationships. But the overall conceptual model (in regards to this specific perceptual variable) was not supported because the mother’s vigilance towards identifying deviancy was not explained by her affective-cognitive defensiveness. On the other hand, there was a pattern of Significant results that did conform to the three requirements listed above. The MCDB were more reactive to child misbehavior and the results indicated that the affective-cognitive defensiveness did have a moderate association with higher levels of reactivity to deviancy. The organizational qualities of the maternal recollections appeared related to the mother’s "deviancy-discrimination schema” involved in the evaluation of the child’s behaviors. 203 In other words, the mother’s childhood recollections, via the construct of a mental model, appeared to play a role in her affect-modulation. It had been proposed that chronically higher levels of reactivity may contribute to an irritable family atmosphere and an ineffective disciplinary style, ultimately impacting on the child’s behavioral status. Hence, these findings do support the overall conceptual model suggesting that the mother’s own childhood experiences have an influence, via her parent-child mental model, on her current parenting style. Clearly, support for the overall conceptualization was mixed. The mother’s parent-child mental model was associated with her affect-modulation and a decreased capacity to correctly identify child behavior. Furthermore, a defensive organization of the mother’s childhood recollections was related to the psychopathology of the child. However, an understanding of the specific nature of this connection between the mother’s mental model and the child’s conduct disturbance does remain elusive and in need of further research. The MCDB vigilance towards identifying deviancy, an important characteristic of their perceptual style, is not explained by the affective- cognitive organization of their childhood recollections. Obviously, the conceptual model needs to be expanded in order to accommodate other significant factors/variables and/or potential interaction effects. Given the equivocal nature of these results, the impact and implications of this study need to be qualified. The greatest impact of this research is in terms of its methodological and conceptual contributions. This study has introduced several useful research instruments. While these instruments, (e.. g, CBS, CBRSI, CBRSII, and the 204 AAQ and the accompanying scoring system) are in need of continued development and further refinement, they nevertheless proved to be useful research tools. Of particular significance is the use of the AAQ, which has some definite advantages over the in-depth interview. Within the domain of attachment-theory, there is currently much interest in exploring the significance of maternal recollections in regards to current parenting. Establishing the effectiveness and usefulness of the AAQ is a potentially important methodological contribution to an already active area of research. Furthermore, this study has made a conceptual contribution which may have applicability to a wide range of research interests. Even though the specific dynamics of the connection remain elusive, nevertheless, this study offers supportive evidence for the construct of an internal mental model which serves as mediator between past events and current functioning. There is a diversity of research perspectives interested in exploring the coherence of behavior patterns across generations, (e.g., Belsky & Pensky, 1988; Simon, Whitbeck, Conger and Chyi-In, 1991; Wahler & Dumas, 1986). Many researchers are investigating behavioral continuity across generations, with particular interest in destructive and/or abusive behavior patterns (e.g., Kaufman & Zigler, 1987). It is very likely that an understanding of the way in which past events are encoded and mentally represented (i.e., organized, processed, and integrated) may be an important explanatory factor in regards to the intergenerational transmission of various behaviors. APPENDICES APPENDIX A LETTER DISTRIBUTED TO MOTHERS 205 Dear Mothers: lam offering FREE attendance to a PARENTING SKILLS WORKSHOP in exchange for your participation in a study involving mothers of boys ages 6-11 years-old. I am a Child and Family Therapist in private practice and completing my graduate studies at Michigan State University. I will be conducting this study, which is part of the requirements for my Ph.D., under the close supervision of Dr. Robert Caldwell, Professor of Psychology at MSU. The present study will explore some important factors involved in parent—child interactions, especially those between mother and son. As most of you are already aware, the mother- child relationship is of critical importance to a child’s growth and development. Parent-child interactions, however, are a two-way street; the quality of these interactions can have a clearly noticeable effect not only on the child, but also on the parents, siblings, and the overall family atmosphere. It is the goal of this study to expand our current knowledge and understanding regarding the possible effects of mother-child relationships. I am writing this letter to personally ask you to participate in this study. I am attempting to locate moMerg who goneioer Meir son go be e Lieh evior problem In Me home, as well as mI r .1 Ira' =._I a ir I’n IIeh- viI ”I II III In N 1!'Vh_= -ver _Ix,‘ Participation in the study in quite simple. Participation will involve meeting for me session lasting approximately three to four hours. At this session you will participate with a group of mothers asked to fill out some rating scales and a questionnaire. All the information obtained will remain strictly confidential. If you are interested and want further details mun-1m. Past experiences suggest that participating in the study may prove to be an interesting, thought provoking, and personally valuable experience. Plus, as a token of my appreciation for your participation, you will have your choice of either a S1599 remuneration, or FREE ADMI'I'TANCE to a PARENTINQ SKILLS WORKSHQP. The half-day workshop will emphasize increasing child management skills and is designed to aide and offer some specific suggestions in dealing with your son’s annoying and disturbing behaviors at home. PLEASE CALL 351-7810 if you are interested in participating in this study and attending the Parenting Skills Workshop. Feel free to call even if you just want to obtain further information. Your participation in this study will be greatly appreciated. I look forward to speaking to you soon. Sincerely , Jerry S. Adams, M.A. ***0ver*** APPENDIX B PHONE INTERVIEW AND FACT SHEET 206 INITIAL PH NE INTERVIEW Thank you for calling about the study. What school does your son go to? How old is your son? What is his date of birth? "The next question, which has four choices, is designed to help in the determination of whether or not you regard your son to be a behavior problem in the home. Please listen carefully to the complete question and then pick the me choice that best describes how you currently regard yours son’s behavior. Choice 1. I consider my son to have fairly serious behavior problems at home. Choice 2. I consider my son to have mild behavior problems at home. Choice 3. I consider my son to basically M have any behavior problems at home; what you’d expect for his age. Choice 4. I consider my son to definitely LL91 have any behavior problems at home; he is very well behaved. " If 1 or 2 ask for some examples. Is you son currently seeing a therapist or counselor? Has he attended therapy in the past? Is anyone in the family seeing a counselor? Has anyone in the family seen a counselor in the past? Who? When? Explain their overall involvement in the study; what their participation will entail. Include information about the feedback sheet immediately after data collection and the written report once the data is analyzed. Explain the three questionnaires, and the consent form, that will be sent in the mail. Ask about the Workshop or financial remuneration? Best times to schedule research session? Any questions they may have. Name(spelling) and address. Thank them for their interest and participation. 207 INITIAL PHONE CONTACT Preliminary Code Mother’s Name: Phone Number: Address: Best times to contact: Where did she hear about study: Son’s DOB: Age: Description of Son’s Behavior: Conduct—disturbed Normal Examples: Family Members: Is anyone currently in TX? Hx: Hx of TX: Her Questions: Best times to schedule session: Tentative plan: Workshop Money Other notes: APPENDIX C CONSENT LETTER 208 Dear Mother: I would like to personally thank you very much for deciding to participate in the Mother-Child Behavior Project. I think you will find that it will be a worthwhile experience. I am writing this letter to welcome your participation in the study, to briefly review, as we discussed on the phone, the purpose of the study, and to have you fill out some preliminary screening information, including the Consent to Participate Form. (For those of you who already received a letter sent home through your son’s school, the first page of this letter will repeat much of the information already presented.) Let me briefly review the purpose of the study and the nature of your participation. The present study will explore some important factors involved in parent-child interactions, especially those between mother and son. As most of you are already aware, the mother-child relationship is of critical importance to a child’s growth and development. The recent emphasis on the teaching of parenting skills highlights the importance of the parent’s role in term of parent-child interactions. Parent-child interactions, however, are a two-way street; the quality of these interactions can have a clearly noticeable effect not only on the child, but also on the parents, siblings, and the overall family atmosphere. It is the goal of this study to expand our current knowledge and understanding regarding the possible effects of mother-child relationships. At the end of the research session I will be giving you a feedback sheet describing the research in greater detail. As soon as the study is completed, the general results and implications will be shared with all participating mothers. Finally, for those of you participating in the Parenting Skills Workshop, a more in-depth description of the ideas and concepts that form the basis for this study will be presented. The design of the study is quite simple and straightforward. Participation will involve meeting for we session lasting approximately three to four hours. At this session you will be part of a group of mothers that will be asked to fill out three different questionnaires. The first two involve the labelling and rating of a wide variety of child behaviors, and the third is a questionnaire regarding your own background. As can be expected, all information obtained will be held strictly confidential. Your name will not appear on any of the research material. The questionnaires will be coded to protect the identity of each participant. As a token of my appreciation for your participation in the study, I am offering you either a $15.00 remuneration, or attendance to the half-day workshop on Parenting Skills. The workshop will emphasize training in child management skills and is designed to aide and offer specific suggestions in dealing with your son’s annoying 209 and disturbing behaviors at home. Overall, the Workshop will attempt to meet the specific needs and interests of the participating mothers. Attached to this cover letter you will find the Cengnt M Pagflcinate Form. Also enclosed, you will find ( 1) the Child Behavier Cheeklist, (2) the Emily W, (3) the Preliminaey Questions, and (4) a pre-addressed retum envelope. PLEASE SIGN THE CONSENT FORM AND FILL OUT THE THREE QUESTIONNAIRES. After you have completed the three questionnaires, please mail them, along with the signed Consent Form, back to me in the envelope provided. As a reminder, please (19 net put your name on any of the questionnaires. As you will notice, the forms are coded so that all the information will remain confidential. Your personal CODE NUMBER is After I receive the completed forms, I will be calling you to set up the appointment for the group session. If you have any questions in the meantime, please feel free to call me at 351-7810. Thanks again for your interest and participation in the study. I’ll be speaking to you soon. Sincerely, Jerry S. Adams, M.A. Signed I Date; 210 N T P IPA RM I have freely consented to participate in a scientific study being conducted by Jerry S. Adams, under the supervision of Dr. Robert Caldwell, Professor of Psychology at Michigan State University. The nature of the study has been explained to me and I understand the explanation and what my participation will involve. I understand that by participating in this study I am entitled to either a $15.00 compensation or participation in a workshop directed toward increasing Parenting Skills. I understand that I am free not to participate at all, not to answer certain questions and/or to discontinue my participation in the study at any time without penalty. I understand that the results of the study will be held in strict confidence and I will remain anonymous in any report of the research findings. Within these restrictions, results of the study will be made available to me at my request. I understand that if there are any questions or concerns that arise as a result of participation in this study, that I can contact Jerry Adams, at (517) 351- 7810. Signed by: Date: APPENDIX D FEEDBACK TO PARTICIPANTS 211 FEEDBACK TO PARTICIPANIS Researcher: Jerry Adams, M.A. Supervisor: Robert Caldwell, Ph.D. The aim of this research project is to examine the relationship between a mother’s current parenting style and her own childhood memories and experiences. It is general knowledge that we all learn from our own past experiences. For better or for worse, our past experiences have an influence on our current behavior. This is very much the case in terms of the ways in which we engage in our parental responsibilities. A mother’s current parenting style is significantly influenced by the ways her own parents behaved and performed their parental responsibilities. For example, most of us have entered adulthood and the role of parenting determined _th to behave or engage in certain behaviors that we did not like in our parents. We have all heard parents say things like, "my mother was too strict and I would never be that way with my own child." On the other hand, there are many of our own childhood experiences that we would like to provide for our own children. For example, ”I try to be as patient with my child as my mother was with me. " It is in this way that a mother’s current parenting style can be influenced by her own childhood experiences. The current research is intended to further explore this relationship between a mother’s childhood memories and her current parenting style. One very important aspect of parenting style has to do with how a parent defines and evaluates child behavior. The ways in which a mother defines a child’s behavior is obviously an important factor influencing the nature and quality of the parent-child interaction. Hence, the current study is an attempt to understand the relationship between a mother’s childhood memories and which behaviors a mother thinks are negative, inappropriate and worthy of punishment and which behaviors are positive, appropriate and thus worthy of praise. I would like to thank you for your time, interest, and participation in this study. After the data is analyzed and the study is completed, I will be mailing out a summary regarding the general results and implications. For those of you who have signed up for the Parenting Skills Workshop, I look forward to our continuing the discussion of these ideas, as well as other concepts and specific suggestions related to child management. Finally, it is possible that you may feel unsettled about the memories and information that you recalled today. If these feeling do not quickly subside and continue to be upsetting, or if you have general comments and questions regarding the study, please feel free to call me, Jerry Adams, at 351-7810. APPENDIX E FAMILY INFORMATION SHEET Ple‘ (_‘a 34" p—n. 2 12 FAMILY INF RMATION EI‘ Please answer all questions as completely as possible. 1. Mother’s Date of Birth: Father’s Date of Birth: Son’s Date of Birth: 2. How many children are there in you current family? Please list the children’s ages: Boys: Girls: 3. Mother’s relationship to son? (Please check one) Natural Parent Step-Parent Other (Please Specify) How many years have mother and son been living together? 4. What is your highest level of education completed? (Please be specific, e. g., one year of junior college) 5. State your type of employment. (Please be specific, e. g., English teacher at Junior High School) 6. What is your husbands highest level of education completed? (Please be specific, e. g., one year of junior college) 7. State your husband’s type of employment. (Please be specific, e. g., English teacher at Junior High School) 8. Please state the approximate annual family income. This should include both parents’ combined income. Thank You. APPENDIX F FAMILY TOGETHERNESS QUESTIONNAIRE - ..\\ 21 3 FAMILY TOGETHERNESS QUESTIONNAIRE Instructions: This brief questionnaire is designed for parents with one or more children in order to give you an opportunity to describe your family. There are no right or wrong answers. Each question has four possible answers, and you simply mark the square under what you believe to be the best answer for each question. The four possible options are (1) very untrue for our family; (2) fairly untrue for our family; (3) Fairly true for our family; and (4) very true for our family. You may feel that some statements are very true for some family members and less true or untrue for others. Judge each statement in terms of most family members. Remember, we would like to know what your family seems like to you. Do not try to figure out what other people might think of your family, but give us yen; impressions of your family. Be as careful and honest as you can, and be sure to answer all 5 of the questions. l 2 3 4 Statement Very Fairly Fairly Very untrue untrue true true for our for our for our for our family family family family 1. Family members find it hard to get away from each other. 2. It is difficult for family members to take time away from the family. 3. Family members feel pressure to spend most free time together. 4. Family members feel guilty if they want to spend time alone. 5. It seems like there is never any place to be alone in our house. APPENDIX G INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE CHILD BEHAVIOR SCENES or 1 diff the YOL thel posi you is e: film the b oldi- For e 214 INSTRUCTIONS FQR THE CHILD BEHAVIQR SCEEES The following stories are designed to help study the different ways that parents follow or keep track of their child’s behaviors, and how parents vary in the way they judge different child behaviors. Clearly, the way in which a parent both identifies and defines the appropriateness of their child’s behavior is an important part of parenting. You will be asked to read several short stories involving boys that are interacting in a variety of situations. The children will be interacting with their peers, their teachers and their families. You will be asked to identify those behaviors you think are desirable and positive and to identify those behaviors that you think are negative and undesirable. As you think about the behaviors as negative or positive, try to imagine that your own son is engaged in the behavior that you are reading about. W As you read through the stories, please underline with the w Pen all those child behaviors that you consider negative, undesirable, aversive, hurtful, and/or inappropriate. INSIIIBCCTIQNS; As you read through the stories, please underline with the CM pen all those child behaviors that you consider positive, desirable, helpful, appropriate or prosocial. Part of the task involves being able to recognize different segments of child behavior. The boys in the stories will be engaged in a wide variety of behaviors. It will be important to read carefully and pay attention to what the boys do, what they think, how the boys react and respond, what they say, and how they feel. All these are examples of different child behaviors. For example: Bob walks home from school. Tom, one of his friends, calls out: "see you tomorrow." On Bob’s way home, he crosses the street. He is looking for something on the ground. Bob picks up a rock and throws the rock into the woods. This example has the following behavioral segments: 1 Bob walked home from school. 2 Tom called out, "see you tomorrow." 3 Bob crossed the street. 4. Bob was looking for something on the ground. 5 Bob picked up the rock. 6 Bob threw the rock into the woods. 215 It is very important that you underline the entire behavioral segment that you have identified. The stories are full of many different examples of child behaviors. You may decide that a sentence has more than one behavior and therefore it is important that each behavior that you think is either negative or positive be completely underlined. Past experience has shown that mothers go about this task in many different ways. It may be helpful to let you know that there are no right or wrong answers. You are not being tested. It is most important that you just be yourself and identify only those behaviors (as positive or negative) that make sense to you. The study is interested in what you, as a mother, think about different child behaviors. So ...... relax, take your time, and please read carefully. Firm reminger: underline with RED all those child behaviors you think are negative and underline in GREEN all those child behaviors that you think are positive. (If you need to review the definition of positive and negative behaviors, please refer to the above paragraphs marked as INSTRUCTIQNS. If you have any further questions, please feel free to ask the researcher. If you have no further questions, you are now ready to begin reading the stories. PLEASE TURN TO THE NEXT PAGE. APPENDIX H THE EXPERTS’ FINAL DETERMINATION OF THE POSITIVE (P), NEGATIVE (N), OR NEUTRAL (BLANK) VALUE FOR EACH BEHAVIOR IN THE CHILD BEHAVIOR SCENES 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 220. 21. 216 AFTER S HOOL Bobby says goodbye to the teacher ........................ He picks up his book bag ................................ and then he starts to walk home .......................... On his way home, he sees a younger boy across the street. and Bobby notices that the boy looks upset ............... He walks over to the younger boy ......................... He realizes the boy is his neighbor Tom .................. Tom is crying ............................................ Brad, an older student, is teasing Tom for crying ........ He is pushing and shoving Torn ............................ Bobby asks Brad to stop teasing Tom ...................... This doesn’t work, so Bobby threatens Brad ............... Bobby throws Brad’s books into the street ................ Brad picks up his books ................................. and he walks away ........................................ Tom thanks Bobby for helping him ............................ Tom then asks Bobby to come to his house for a snack ..... Bobby says, "thanks, but I can’t. I need to get home"... Bobby is lying about nwding to go home .................. Bobby walks across the street ............................ and enters the little store .............................. zH‘FH Z (scene. 1) Si If.) 217 The story owner greets Bobby 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. and Bobby responds by saying, "hello Mr. Niles. How are you?" ..................... Bobby stands and looks at all the different candy bars... He looks to see where the store owner is standing ........ Then, he quickly puts a piece of candy into his jacket pocket ..... and he walks out the door ................................ He then eats the stolen candy bar ........................ When he gets home, his mother is happy to see him ........ He gives her a hug ....................................... Then he walks across the room into the kitchen ........... He is starting to feel guilty about stealing the candy... He decides to take out the trash ......................... Mother seems surprised at his behavior. 33. He then tells his mother about stealing the candy ........ ._2__ She informs him that he will have to miss his favorite activity for a week. 34. 35. 36. Bobby says: "that makes me really mad." .................. He starts to whine : "that just isn’t fair!" ............. He kicks the chair in anger .............................. His mother tells him to go to his room. 37. 38. 39. 40. As he walks away, he swears at her ....................... Then, he goes to his room as asked ....................... After some time, he apologizes for swearing .............. He promises to never swear again ......................... 218 ETTIN READY R DINNER 1. John asks his mother, "what time will dinner be ready?".. She says in 20 minutes. 2. John asks what they are having for dinner ................ His mother answers: "We will be having salad, peas and meatloaf." 3. Paul, John’s brother, walks into the room ................ 4. Paul interrupts the conversation ......................... 5. He starts complaining that he hates peas ................. 6. Paul then starts whining: "I want to eat spaghetti!" ..... His mother impatiently yells at him to stop whining. 7. Paul starts crying ....................................... Mother says: "quit being a baby and stop crying!" 8. Paul stops crying ........................................ 9. He then says to his mother: "It hurts my feelings when you yell at me." ............. She replies, "don’t be so sensitive". As mother walks away, 10. Paul says, "you know, sometimes it’s really hard to talk to you." .................. Dad comes home from work. He calls out, "hello, I’m home." 11. Paul greets dad at the door .............................. 12. He gives his father a hug and a kiss ..................... 13. Paul tells his father that mother has hurt his feelings. Z 2 Z M II .6 Mot Mother calls out from the kitchen and asks for volunteers to set the table for dinner. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 219 John says, "it’s not my turn to set the table, I did it last night." ....................... Paul says: "Alright, I’ll do it." ........................ He begins to set the table for dinner .................... While setting the table, Paul drops a glass that breaks all over the floor ........................ John laughs at Paul for breaking the glass ............... John says, "I’m going to tell mother you broke the glass." John runs to the kitchen to tell his mother that Paul has broken a glass ..................... Paul explains that it was an accident .................... He apologizes for breaking the glass ..................... Mother accepts his apology. 23. 24. 25 . 26. 27. 28. 29. Paul tells his brother, "it makes me mad when you try to get me in trouble. " ............. John responds by grinning ................................. Paul says to his brother John: " you are an idiot! " ....... and then Paul hits his brother on the arm ................ Paul gets a broom to sweep up the broken glass ........... He cleans up the broken glass ............................ He finishes setting the table ............................ Mother announces that dinner is ready. 2 Z Z 1. 220 DINNER Todd is watching TV ...................................... Mother informs Todd that dinner is ready. 2. Todd says, "I’ll go upstairs and tell everyone that dinner is ready." ....... Mother says, " thank you Todd." 3. Todd goes upstairs ....................................... The door to his father’s bedroom is closed. 4. Todd knocks on the bedroom door .......................... His father calls out, "come in." 5. Todd enters his father’s room and says, "dad, dinner is ready." ...... Dad responds, "thank you son, I’ll be right down". 6. Todd then goes to Steve’s room, his older brother ........ The door is closed. 7. Todd opens the door without knocking ..................... 8. Steve yells out, "get out of here you creep!" ............ 9. Steve is secretly looking at a magazine of naked women... 10. He offers Todd a dollar if he doesn’t tell about the magazine ...... 11. Todd takes the dollar ................................... 12. and he promises not to tell his parents about Steve’s magazine ....... 13. Todd remembers to wash his hands before dinner .......... (scene.3) 221 14. He reminds his brother to wash his hands too ............. P Dad is also washing. They all go downstairs for dinner. 15. Steve volunteers to help serve dinner .................... P 16. Todd sits down at the table .............................. 17. Steve asks Todd what he wants to drink ................... P 18. Todd answers that he wants pop ........................... Dad says, "not again, please drink something else tonight." 19. Todd says, "that makes me mad when I can’t have what I want to drink." ....... Father reminds him that there is a limit on how much pop they can drink. 20. Todd complains "that isn’t fair!" ........................ N Dad responds: "life isn’t always fair son." Mother asks Todd to pick something else to dfink. 21. Todd begins to whine: "I’m not going to drink anything! And I’m not going to eat!" ............. N Dad ignores Todd and states, "I’m very hungry. Steve, what did mother make for dinner?" 22. Steve answers his father, "we are having salad, chicken and mixed vegetables.".... 23. Steve brings the food to the table ....................... P 24. Todd is sitting at the table pouting ..................... N Mother asks father, "how was your day dear?" Father starts to talk about his day. 25 . Todd interrupts the conversation ......................... N Mother and father continue to talk and finish their conversation. 26. Steve asks, "dad, will you please pass the chicken." ..... P 222 27. Steve takes a piece of chicken ........................... 28. and he offers, "does anyone else want some more chicken?" ....... 29. Todd says "no thanks." ................................... 30. Todd has stopped pouting ................................. 31. Todd eats all of the food on his plate ................... 32. Todd tells his mother: "I really liked the chicken. It’s my favorite!" ..................... 33. Steve asks if he can be excused from the table ..... 34. He says that he is going up to his room to start his homework... Father tells him that he may leave. On his way upstairs, 35. Steve teases the dog with a piece of chicken ........... Father then asks Todd to help clear the table. 36. Todd brings all the dishes from the table into the kitchen ............. 37. He then begins to load the dishwasher .................. When this is finished, 38. Todd tells his father, "I better begin my homework now.". 39. He asks if it will be alright to do his homework at the dinning room table... 40. Todd asks his father for help with his math homework... Mother finishes straightening up in the kitchen. 223 (scene.4) AR THEHUE Mother and father are sitting and relaxing in the den. There is a fire burning in the fireplace. Their four boys are doing different things around the house. 1. Peter is reading a book .................................. P 2. Roger is working on his model airplane ................... P 3. Mike is outside playing basketball ...................... P 4. Jake is wandering around the house ....................... __ 5. Jake walks into the kitchen .............................. __ 6. He pockets the $1.00 that his father left on the kitchen counter ...... N 7. He starts to look through his parents’ mail .............. N 8. He takes a cookie out of the cookie jar .................. __ 9. and he goes into the living room to eat it ............... 10. Jake yells towards the den: "there’s nothing to do!" ..... N 11. Jake is bored ............................................ 12. and he proceeds to pick leaves off some plants in the living room ...... N There is a book of matches on the table. 13. Jake lights a match ...................................... N 14. He throws the burning match into the ashtray ............. 15. He looks through some magazines on the coffee table ...... 16. He reads an articles on ocean cruises .................... 17. He then stacks the magazines neatly ...................... P 18. Jake looks out the front window .......................... 224 19. He watches his brother and friends playing basketball on the driveway ........ Mother walks past the living room and sees Jake staring out the window. She says impatiently: "why don’t you find something to do?" She goes into the kitchen and calls out, "Jake, the lid is off the cookie jar. Did you have one?" 20. Jake replies, "nope, it wasn’t me." ...................... N 21. Jake walks back into the kitchen ......................... Mother is pouring a glass of beer to bring to dad in the den. The phone rings and mother goes to answer it. The call is apparently for her and she starts talking. 22. Jake stands in the kitchen for a minute .................. 23. and then reaches over and takes a sip of dad’s beer ...... N 24. He makes a face at the taste ............................. The family cat comes into the kitchen. 25 . Jake pulls the cat’s tail ................................ N 26. Then Jake sits on the floor .............................. _ 27. He pets the cat for awhile ............................... P 28. He picks up the cat ...................................... 29. and then he holds the cat in his lap ..................... Mother gets off the phone and returns to the kitchen. 30. Jake says, "I heard a good joke the other day." .......... P 31. He proceeds to tell his mother the joke .................. P Mother laughs. 32. Jake feels good because his mother liked the joke ........ P Mother musses his hair in an affectionate way. 33. He pushes her hand away ................................. 225 34. and he says, "stop it, mom." ............................. As mother leaves the kitchen she says, "why don’t you fwd the cat." 35 . Jake says "sure, no problem. I’ll do it right now." ...... 36. He asks, "should I give the cat the canned stuff. " ....... Mother replies, "alright, but only half of it." 37. Jake feeds the cat ........................................ 38. He follows his mother’s instructions ..................... 39. He lays on the kitchen floor ............................. 40. Jake watches the cat eat ................................. 41. He then decides to give the cat some milk ................ The cat drinks the milk and then strolls off into the other room. 42. Jake leaves the milk carton on the counter ............... 43. Jake goes into his bedroom ............................... 44. He begins to read a comic book ........................... 45. He then decides to go see if he can play some basketball. 46. He puts on his tennis shoes .............................. 47. He asks his brother Mike if he can join the game ......... They need an extra player to make the teams even. 48. Jake is happy because he will be able to play basketball. 49. He is a good basketball player ........................... 50. Jake scores a lot of points during the game .............. Nonetheless, his team looses. P 226 51. Jake is a good sport about loosing ....................... P 52. He tells the other team, "good game guys!" ............... P The other boys agree it was a good game. After deciding to play again tomorrow, they all say good-bye and go home. 53. Mike, however, is complaining about loosing .............. N 54. Mike accuses the other team of cheating on the score ..... N 55. Jake attempts to console his brother ..................... P 56. He says to Mike: "chill out, it’s only a game. We’ll beat ’em next time. We’re going to play again tomorrow." ................................. P 57. Mike starts to cry about how much he was fouled during the game ....... N 58. Jake is becoming annoyed with Mike’s behavior ............ 59. He shouts at Mike, "shut up!! Quit being such a poor sport!" ..... N 60. Mike throws a small tantrum by yelling and pounding his fists on the garage door... N 61. Jake goes back into the house ............................ 62. He is feeling tired ...................................... 63. He takes some root beer from the refrigerator ............ 64. He sneaks another cookie from the cookie jar ............. 65. He takes the root beer and the cookie to his bedroom ..... 66. He eats the snack on his bed ............................. 67. He decides to take a shower .............................. ll|||21 ll 68. After the shower, Jake falls asleep on his bed ........... Mother wakes Jake and tells him that they are all going out for pizza. 227 69. At first he is grumpy for being awaken ................... 70. Then, Jake is excited .................................... 71. He really likes pizza .................................... APPENDIX I CHILD BEHAVIOR RATING SCALES I/II 228 HILD BEHAVI R RATIN AL The Child Behavior Rating Scale was devised to examine how parents judge certain child behaviors. The purpose of the scale is to obtain ygu; judgement, as a parent, of both negative and positive child behaviors. Section I emphasizes child misbehavior. Section II emphasizes positive or desirable child behaviors. 53119111 Section I of the Child Behavior Rating Scale is designed to help us understand how parents rate the degree of misconduct of certain negative behaviors. In this section of the Child Behavior Rating Scale you will be asked about your own judgement regarding the seriousness of misbehavior. You will be asked to read through a list of child behaviors and to give your own opinion as to how important or serious you think each negative behavior really is. For example, most parents would agree that lying to a friend is a negative and undesirable child behavior. On the other hand, parents will disagree when judging the degree of seriousness regarding this misconduct. While some parents would consider a child lying to a friend to be only a slightly serious misbehavior, other parents would consider this a serious or perhaps even an extremely serious misbehavior. It is interesting to point out that even professionals regarded as "experts" in the area of child behavior would disagree about the level of misbehavior. Obviously, there are no right or wrong answers to these evaluations, but rather only differences of judgement and opinion. Specifically, you will be presented with a list containing a variety of primarily negative, undesirable, aversive, hurtful, and/or inappropriate child behaviors. You will be asked to rate each misbehavior in terms of it’s seriousness. Each behavior on the list is performed by a boy. You are asked to imagine that the boy is the same age as your own son. INSTRUCTIONS CONTINUE ON THE NEXT PAGE PLEASE TURN TO THE NEXT PAGE 229 Each behavior will be rated on the following six-point scale: Slight Serious Misbehavior Misbehavior O 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not at all a Moderate Extremely Misbehavior Serious Misbehavior W Please rate each behavior on the list according to this scale. Please rate each behavior separately. Please show your rating of each behavior by circling the appropriate number that corresponds with your judgement. For example, the actual list of child behaviors will look like this: Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 0 1 2 3 4 5 The boy lied to his teacher. 0 l 2 3 4 5 Johnny started spitting at the girl. 0 1 2 3 4 5 Bobby was talking too loudly at the dinner table. Lets look at the first behavior as an example. If you decide that the boy’s lying to his teacher is not really a misbehavior at all, then you would circle the number "0". If you think that his lying is only slightly negative or a slightly serious misbehavior then you would rate the behavior a "2", and you would circle the number "2". On the other hand, if you thought that lying was a serious or extremely serious misbehavior then you would circle either "4" or "5" depending on your judgement. The ratings of "2" or "3" are in the moderate range, with "3" obviously leaning towards the more serious misbehavior. Once again, you would circle the number that best reflects your actual judgement. Remember, there are no right or wrong answers and you are not being tested. The purpose of the Child Behavior Rating Scale is to help in obtaining your opinion. Please read each behavior carefully and circle the number that best reflects your judgement of that behavior. If you have any further questions now is the time to ask the researcher. If not, you are ready to begin. Please wait until the researcher announces: "Please turn to the next page and begin". 230 HILD BEHAVI R RA ALE SECTION! Slight Serious Misbehavior Misbehavior O l 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not at all a Moderate Extremely Misbehavior Serious Misbehavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 The boy lied to his teacher. Johnny started spitting at the girl. Steve reminded his parents that the movie started in 30 minutes. Bobby was talking too loudly at the dinner table. He never told his mother about the phone message. The boy was interrupting his parents’ conversation. Pete forgot to lock the door of the house as asked. The boys were trying to smoke cigarettes in their secret club house. The boy said to his younger sister: "nobody loves you cuz you’re a little brat." He came home 20 minutes later than he was told to. He was screaming as loudly as he could at his mother. He turned in his book report two weeks late. Todd threatened the other boy that he would kill his cat. Chuck was listening in on his mother’s phone conversation. Fred was blasting his radio. Al decided to break the mirror in the school bathroom. The boy lied to his mother about having no homework because he wanted to watch TV. Bill told his father that he disagreed with many of his opinions. Sam refused to do his chores around the house. The boy was whining because he couldn’t have any ice cream. 231 Section I page, 2 Slight Serious Misbehavior Misbehavior O l 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not at all a Moderate Extremely Misbehavior Serious Misbehavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 Bob forged his mother’s signature on his report card. Rick was in the backyard lighting matches. Ted was secretly looking at a magazine of naked women. The boy interrupted the teacher without raising his hand. He stole some money from his father’s wallet. John was urinating on the living room floor. He threw a rock and broke the neighbor’s window because he was mad at them. He was very angry when he said to his mother: "you’re a mean women!" Tom’s room was messy because his clothes and toys were all over the floor. The boys were smoking a marijuana cigarette. He told his mother that the chicken was overcooked. He was calling people on the phone and hanging up on them. He quickly opened the car door and stole a few music tapes. Gary opened his parents’ bedroom door without knocking. Sam was bored and no one was home. He decided to see what beer tasted like and he drank a can of beer from the refrigerator. Even though his lights were off in the bedroom, Phil was using his ear phones to secretly listen to his radio. He fed the dog an hour later than he was supposed to. Randy was snooping in his parent’s room looking for his upcoming birthday gift. He was shooting the BB gun at the birds in the tree. ll: Pl 232 Section I page, 3 Slight Serious Misbehavior Misbehavior 0 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not at all a Moderate Extremely Misbehavior Serious Misbehavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 The boy told his father that certain rules around the house made him angry. When the teacher turned around, he shot a rubber-band at the teacher’s back. This year Dave never wished his mother a happy birthday. The boy didn’t wash his hands before dinner. The boy was kissing a girl on the way home after school. Paul copied his friends homework because he didn’t do it himself. After the teacher told him to sit down, Ted made an obscene gesture to the teacher. He said to his father: "sometimes you can really be a jerk!” He took out a knife and threatened his teacher. Torn pretended that the little plastic bag was full of marijuana. After being pushed and shoved by Nick, Chris started swearing at him. Steve was "playing Doctor" with the girl next door. He didn’t put the butter back into the refrigerator. Jack was playing with his father’s rifle. He said to his father: "I’ll be out playing for the rest of the afternoon." Jack told his mother: "I hate you!" He continued to watch TV and ignored his mother’s pleas for him to go to bed. Setti Not 2 list Pleas 233 Section I page, 4 Slight Serious Misbehavior Misbehavior O 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not at all a Moderate Extremely Misbehavior Serious Misbehavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 Ned’s feelings were hurt and he told his parents, "I’m never going to speak to either one of you ever again!” On his way home from school, the boy took a rock and scratched the side of a parked car. He persistently kept nagging his mother to take him shopping for some new jeans. Whenever Sam’s mother asked him about taking out the trash he would say, "Gee, I forgot." The boy asked if he could stay up late to finish watching his favorite TV show. He complained to the teacher that she was giving too much homework. Paul was sitting at the dinner table playing with his food rather than eating it. YOU HAVE NOW COMPLETED sscnoN r OF THE CHILD BEHAVIOR RATING SCALE PLEASE TURN THE PAGE AND READ THE INSTRUCTIONS FOR SECTION H 234 HILD BEHAVI R RATIN AL Instructions for Section II In Section I of the Child Behavior Rating Scale you were asked to give your judgement regarding negative child behaviors. Section II will now emphasize positive or prosocial child behaviors. You should begin the Instructions for Section 11 only after you have completely rated all the behaviors listed in Section I. The Instructions for Sections H assume that you are now already very familiar with the procedure and format of the Child Behavior Rating Scale. Section H is basically the same as the first section, except now you will be rating different behaviors in terms of their W. W In Section II of the Child Behavior Rating Scale you will be asked about your own judgement regarding the desirability of certain positive child behaviors. Specifically, you will be presented with a list containing a variety of primarily positive, desirable, helpful, appropriate and/or prosocial child behaviors. You will be asked to rate each positive behavior in terms of it’s desirability. Each behavior on the list is performed by a boy. You are asked to imagine that the boy is the same age as your own son. Each behavior will be rated on the following six-point scale: I. _ ' .0 NO I I3! DIFFEREN ls '!___ \L RA I a I-" Slightly Highly Desirable Desirable 0 l 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ I \ Not a Clearly Moderate Extremely Desirable Desirable Behavior Behavior Remember, there are no right or wrong answers and you are not being tested. The purpose of Child Behavior Rating Scale is only to obtain your opinion. Please read each behavior carefully and circle the number that best reflects your judgement of that behavior. If you have any further questions please raise your hand and ask the researcher. If you have no questions, then you are ready to begin Section II. Please turn to the next page and begin Section II 235 BEHAVI R RAT G AL E TI N 11 INSTRUCTIONS Please rate each behavior on the list according to this scale. Please rate each behavior separately. Please show your rating of each behavior by circling the appropriate number that corresponds with your judgement. Slightly Highly Desirable Desirable 0 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not a Clearly Moderate Extremely Desirable Desirable Behavior Behavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 The boy woke up early in the morning in order to do his paper route. Pete let his friend know how much he cared about him. Ted put some more sugar in his chocolate drink. Bob helped his blind neighbor walk to school. He wrote down the phone message on the pad where messages were kept. He washed his hands for dinner. George volunteered to help set the table for dinner. Tim asked the teacher if he could turn his report in a few days late. Charles was very excited about a new stamp he added to his collection. Terry decided to watch TV by himself in his room. Carlos was elected president of his church youth group. The boy told his father that he was mad at him for missing his hockey game. During the conversation at dinner, Pete told a joke and everyone laughed. Jesse practiced his musical instrument three days a week. Dan told his parents that his brother was smoking cigarettes. 236 Section H page, 2 Slightly Highly Desirable Desirable 0 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not a Clearly Moderate Extremely Desirable Desirable Behavior Behavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 He called his parents to tell them that he would be a half-hour late. Ted got five dollars for mowing the neighbors lawn. Tom stayed up a half-hour past his bedtime in order to finish his homework. Ben said: "I really don’t like what we are having for dinner. Can I make a sandwich?" He made his mother a nice card for Mothers Day. Stan tried to stop the fight at school. At the dinner table, Ned passed the potatoes to his father. When his mother changed her mind, Paul said "I’m really bummed. I hate it when you do that!" Rick saved enough of his money to buy his favorite album. Clearance spent Sunday afternoon writing letters to his friends from camp. He took the long way home from school because it was such a nice spring day outside. Tyronne gave his dog a bath. Fritz loaned three dollars to his friend from school. Alan tried to get his parents to st0p fighting. Don wouldn’t let his friend copy off of his test paper. The boy asked his parents if they could go to the school play. On Halloween, Stuart got over three pounds of candy in his bag. He helped the younger boys build their tree house. 237 Section 11 page, 3 Slightly Highly Desirable Desirable 0 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not a Clearly Moderate Extremely Desirable Desirable Behavior Behavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 Billy’s discussion with his father about nuclear weapons turned into a heated debate. Buck turned on his night-light before falling asleep. Jeff listened with interest to the stories about his friend’s trip to Mexico. Jerry made toast and juice for his parents in order to surprise them with breakfast. Gil turned the TV volume very low since everyone in the house was still sleeping. Nick announced that he was wearing socks with his shoes. The boy politely asked if they could possibly go somewhere else for dinner since he really didn’t like the food at the restaurant where they were going. Philip told his dad that he was scared about going away to summer camp. Max showered every evening before going to bed. He knew his friend was allergic to chocolate so he bought some different types of candy to share. Justin read a story to his younger sister. Dick was bored watching the cartoons so he went outside. Jim said to his mother, "I wish you’d pay more attention to me." His younger brother teased him and attempted to provoked him, but Mario was able to go outside and walk off his frustrations. Frank’s room was very neat and clean. 238 Section H page, 4 Slightly Highly Desirable Desirable O 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not a Clearly Moderate Extremely Desirable Desirable Behavior Behavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 012345 Drake donated all of this weeks allowance to the firnd for needy children. Mark brushed his teeth every morning before school. He knew his mother was sick, so he cleaned up the entire kitchen. He told his friend that they couldn’t bring their sandwiches to his bedroom because there was no food allowed outside of the kitchen. Even though Sam was really mad and he felt Nick deserved it, Sam stopped himself from swearing at his friend. Tommy told the coach that he needed more encouragement in order to stay on the team. The boy told his mother that he was going to bed early. During the dinner conversation Buck said, "I was really embarrassed today when the teacher asked me a question and I didn’t know the answer." The boy asked if he could stay up late to finish watching his favorite show. He made an effort to keep away from Mike because Mike was always getting into trouble. Dick asked his dad for help with his vocabulary list. Jeff spent the entire afternoon by himself taking an enjoyable walk. Rich signed up at school to have a pen-pal. 239 Section H page, 5 Slightly Highly Desirable Desirable 0 l 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ \ Not a Clearly Moderate Extremely Desirable Desirable Behavior Behavior Please Circle That Number Which Best Reflects Your Judgement of Each Behavior 012345 012345 012345 012345 Carl gave up his bus seat to an older women. Shawn wanted to learn more about his cultural background. Larry decided to stay home and finish reading his book. Brad asked his mother if he could sleep over at his friends house. APPENDIX J ADULT ATTACHMENT QUESTIONNAIRE 240 W This questionnaire is designed to improved our understanding of how parents feel that their own childhood experiences have had an effect on them as parents and as people. The following questions concern your childhood relationships with your parents (or parental figures). Most of the questions will ask about your early relationships with your family and what you think about the ways these relationships might have affected you. The questions are mostly about your childhood, but a few of the questions are about your later years and what is going on right now. To get yourself in the right frame of mind, take a few minutes to think about yourself as a child. Try to imagine the house(s) you lived in and the school(s) you attended. Can you get an image of the physical surroundings, or a mental picture of the neighborhood? Try to remember what your parents and siblings looked like and how they behaved. Try to imagine yourself as far back in time as you can. _ ° V a II III 1.7.! I: I . 5. I. '." .I'I ‘91 II ..I 2. II .I “1.1., 1. man _I J. In fact, it may be easiest for you to give MW of situations or circumstances when filling out the questionnaire. Please be as specific as you can when recalling an event, (e. g., referring to what was done, what was said, how people reacted etc.). Remember, there are no right or wrong answers. The questions are designed to gather information regarding your early relationships with your family. Let yourself be as relaxed as possible so that you may feel free to write down whatever comes to mind. Please do not be concerned with spelling, punctuation, and/or grammar, but rather please answer the questions in as much detail/content as possible. (You may use the backs of pages or additional pages if you wish. But please label these clearly.) The questions are designed to be answered in order. Please do not look ahead in the Questionnaire. Rather, start with Question 1 and when you have W Wk proceed to Question 2. Once a question has been completed, please do not go back and change the answer. Finally, please use a pen and not a pencil. (Please note the time you started the questionnaire, as I will want to know how much time it took to complete.) What time is it now? PLEASE TURN TO THE NEXT PAGE AND BEGIN THE QUESTIONNAIRE 241 1. Starting from as far back as you can remember, please describe your relationships with your parents. (Try to provide specific examples.) 242 2. If you had to choose four adjectives that reflect your ehildhoog eeIaMLrgIIjn with m mether which four would you choose? 3. Now, for e_aM adjective say why you think that particular adjective is a good descriptor of your childhood relationship with your mother. Why did you choose that adjective? (It may be easiest to give a smific example of a time when she did or said a certain thing or acted in a particular way.) Remember that detafl is important. First adjective: Second adjective: Third adjective: 243 Fourth adjective: 4. If you had to pick four adjectives to describe the most important emotional reactions you had to your mother (the most important ways she made you feel or affected my: fleeing), which four would you choose? PI nin ih ’ nhn 244 5. If you had to choose four adjectives that reflect m c_hi_lg_h_qed mm with me father which four would you choose? 6. Now, for _e_a_el_r_ adjective say why you think that particular adjective is a good descriptor of your childhood relationship with your father. Why did you choose that adjective? (It may be easiest to give a smifie exaMQIe of a time when he did or said a certain thing or acted in a particular way.) Remember that detail is important. First adjective: Second adjective: Third adjective: 245 Fourth adjective: 7. If you had to pick four adjectives to describe the most important emotional reactions you had to your father (the most important ways he made you feel or affected 19!: LLIings), which four would you choose? 8. During your childhood it is very likely that you felt closer to either your mother or your father. To which parent did you feel the closest and why? Why wasn’t there this feeling with the other parent? 246 9. When you were upset as a child what would you typically do? These are a series of follow- -up questions concerning how you got comforted as a child. Searching back in your memory as far as you can, W what you would typically do if you were in need of comforting. For each question, please say whether you would tell anyone, and if you did, whom did you tell, and what would that person typically do (e. g. hold you, talk to you, tell you a joke, play a game, ignore you, tease you, etc). 9a. When you were emotionally upset as a child what would you do? Were there "typical" things or events that made you emotionally upset? (e. g., hurt feelings, disappointments, bad dreams, being scared etc.) Please describe W or exaMQIes that you remember. If you did turn to your parents when you were upset, how did they comfort or soothe you? 247 9b. What would happen when you were physically hurt or injured? Please describe some W of being hurt or injured. Did you tell anyone? Whom did you tell? How did they comfort or soothe you? 9c. Finally, what would happen when you were ill as a child. Again, please provide W of when you were ill, what you would do, and how others, specifically your parents, would respond. 248 10. How did you go about getting attention as a child? How did you get your parents to respond and/or attend to you and your needs? (Again, We: will be helpful.) 10B. Did your parents take an interest in your activities and accomplishments? How much importance was placed on your activities and your achievements? (New nrevide smifle examples). 249 10C. Did your parents seem to take more of an interest in your activities and accomplishments than in your emotional concerns and needs? Or, was there a balance between the two? Were there some things about you and/or your activities that interested your parents more than other needs and/or activities? 11. During your childhood, were there any adults, other than your parents, to whom you felt especially close? If so, please describe the relationship and the circumstances, for example, say what their relationship was to you, how much time you spent with them, how old you were at the time, etc. 250 12. During your childhood, were you ever separated from either of your parents for what felt like a long time? If so, for how long? How old were you? What was the reason for the separation? How did you feel about it at the time? 13. When you were a child, were your parents ever threatening to you in any way,--- maybe because they were angry, or for discipline, or maybe just jokingly? For example, did your parents ever threaten to hurt you, to leave you, or send you away? Did they ever call you names and/or say that you were a bad child? Please give Mtge W. Did this happen frequently? How do you think this experience(s) affects you now as an adult? 251 14. Did you ever feel rejected as a young child? Of course, looking back on it now, you may realize it was not really rejection, but this specific question is concerned with whether you ever fielt rejected as a child? How often did you feel rejected? What were the circumstances? How old were you and what did you do? (Again, it may be easiest to give Wales.) 14A. Why do you think your parents did those things? Do you think he/she realized he/she was rejecting you? 252 15. At this point in the questionnaire you have probably shared a lot of childhood memories regarding your relationship with your parents, remembering a variety of specific interactions. In your opinion, why did your parents behave the way they did toward you when you were a child? 16. What effect, if any, did your early experiences and relationships with your parents have on your adult personality? On your adult relationships with friends and lovers? Are there any experiences that you feel were a set-back in your development? 253 17. Have there been many changes in your relationship with your parents (or remaining parent) since childhood? In what way(s) have these relationships changed over the years? In what way(s) have they remained the same? 17A. Describe your current relationship with each of your parents. What is the relationship like for your now as an adult? 254 18. For most of us, there were some aspects of our childhood relationships with our parents that were, to varying degrees, troublesome, conflictual, and/or problematic. Do these issues remain unresolved and still active? Or, have these troublesome aspects of your childhood relationships with your parents been resolved and/or put to rest? Please dflfibe in deMiI the process of how you have dealt with these troublesome or problematic aspects of your childhood relationships. 19. Is there any particular thing which you feel you learned above all from your own childhood experiences? What would you hope your child might have learned (or will learn) from his/her experiences of being parented? 255 Now that you have almost completed the questionnaire, the next two questions focus on your own subjective feelings regarding your ability or inability to remember your own childhood. These questions are concerned with how LIE—Ed about your ability or inability to recall your own childhood experiences. 1. How well do you feel you could remember your own childhood experiences? (Please circle the number that best reflects your own judgment. Do not pick a rating between two numbers.) Vague Memories Clear Memories 0 1 2 3 4 \ \ \ \ \ Basically Unable to Average Extremely Vivid Remember Childhood Ability to Recall Memories with Strong Ability to Recall 2. Now that you have rated your own ability to remember your childhood, how confident are you about this rating? (For example, say that you rated yourself as having only vague memories (#1), how confident are you that you only have vague memories of childhood.) (Please circle the number that best reflects your level of confidence. Do not pick a rating between two numbers.) Slightly Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 \ \ \ \ \ Not at all Fairly Totally Confident Confident 3. Were some aspects of your childhood easier to remember than others? What was easy to remember? And what was difficult to remember? Thank you very much for taking the time to fill out this questionnaire! Please note what time it is now PLEASE CONTINUE TO THE NEXT PAGE. 256 Since this questionnaire is in the continual process of development and modification, it is very important to get your reactions to answering these questions. Please take a few minutes to let me know what it was like for you to answer these questions. Anything you want to share regarding your reactions to the questions will be helpful. (Also, if you have not already done so, please note the amount of time it took to complete the questionnaire.) Finally, if for any reason you are feeling upset or unsettled and would like to talk about your reactions to the various questions, please feel free to call me directly at 351-7319. Leave your name and number and I will get back to you as soon as possible. Thank you very much for your time and cooperation. Jerry Adams LIST OF REFERENCES REFERENCES Achenbach, T.M. , & Edelbrock, C. S. (1978). The classification of child psychopathology: A review and analysis of empirical efforts. Psyehelegiefl Bellep'e, 85, 1275-1301. Anderson, K.E., Lytton, H., & Romney, D. (1986). Mothers’ interactions with normal and conduct-disordered boys: Who affects whom? WM, 22, 604-609. Bauer, W.D., & Twentyman, C. T.(1985). Abusing, neglectful, and comparison mothers’ responses to child-related and one child-related stressors. JeemaLef CenseltM g Me Clinieal Peyehelegy, i3, 335-343. Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Qeyelepmeptal Bulldog» fl, 1-103. Bell, R.Q. (1979). Parent, child, and reciprocal influences. WW, MOO), 821-826. Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Chm WI 5;, 83-96' Belsky, J., & Pensky, E. (1988). Developmental history, personality, and family relationships: toward an emergent family system. In R. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (EdS) WWW (pp 193 217) New York Oxford University Press. Berkowitz, L. (1973). Control of aggression. In B. Caldwell & H. Ricciuti (Eds. ), RviWIfhflwvelmenrth 'IIleIm 1!,0 -._,III (pp. 95- 140). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bloom, BL. (1985). A factor analysis of self-report measures of family functioning. Fmily Preeess, 23(2), 225-239. Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment Theory: Retrospect and prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds. ), Growing points in attachment theory and research, hieeegeapbe ef Me Seei Iety fer Regatta; M C hrle Qevelepment, 511 (1-2 Serial No. 209), 3- 35. Bugental, D.B., & Shennum, W.A. (1984). "Difficult" children as elicitors and targets of adult communication patterns: An atuibutional-behavioral transactional analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, _4_9_ (l , Serial No. 205), 1-70. 257 258 Cahill, M.F. (1978). A search for the meaning of the "difficult" child. moon WILL 12, 18,154. Christensen, A., Phillips, 5., Glasgow, R., & Johnson, S. (1983). Parental characteristics and interactional dysfunction in families with child behavior problems: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, HO), 153-166. Crowell, J. A., & Feldman, S. S. (1988). Mothers’ internal models of relationships and children’s behavioral and developmental status: A study of mother-child interaction. mm L9 1273- 1235 Delfini, L.F., Bernal, M.E., & Rosen, P.M. (1976) Comparison of deviant and normal boys in home settings. In E]. Mash, L.A. Hamerlynck, & L.C. Handy (Eds.), W New Yorszmnner/Mazel. Forehand, R., Brody, G., & Smith, K. (1986). Contributions of child behavior and marital dissatisfaction to maternal perceptions of child maladjustment. mm W. 25.. 4348. Forehand, R. ,King, H, Peed, 8., & Yoder, P. (1975). Mother-child interactions. . Comparison of a non-compliant clinic group and a non-clinic group. Behayio; W21, 13, 79-84. Forehand R., Wells, K.C., & Sturgis, E.T. ( 1978). Predictors of child noncompliant behavior in the home. Journal of Consulm 2 god glinioal Psyohology, fl, 179-180. Frank, S., Jacobson, 8., Hole, C., Justkowski, R., & Huyck, M. (1986). Psychological predictors of parents’ sense of confidence and control and self- versus child-focused gratifications. Developmengl Psychology, _2__2_, 348-355. George, C., Kaplan. N.. & Main. M. (1985)- WWW Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley. Greenberg, M., & Speltz, M. (1988). Attachment and the ontogeny of conduct problems. In J. Belsky & T Nezworski (Eds.), Clinioo; Imoligo'ons of Attoohmont (pp.l77-218). I-Iillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Griest, D.L., & Wells, K.C. (1983). Behavior family therapy with conduct disorders in children. Behavior Thoraoy, 1_4, 37- 53. 259 Grossmann, K., Fremmer—Bombik, E., Rudolph, J ., & Grossmann, KB. (1988). Maternal attachment representations as related to patterns of infant-mother attachment and maternal care during the first year. In R. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds. ), WW (pp 241-262) New York Oxford University Press. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J fP r ' d i P h l , 5;, 511-524. Hetherington, E. M., & Martin, B. (1986). Family factors and psychopathology in children In HC Quay & JHS Worry (EdS) WW ohildhotxl (3rd ed. ). New York. Wiley. Hinde, R. A. (1980). Family influences. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Wm WM (pp. 47-63). London: Heinemann Medical. Holleran, P. A. Littrnan, D. C., Freund, R. D., Schmaling, K. B., & Heeren, J. (19823) ,_ Igle II .III.C I I I; A’Oltn MulI,_II If "._I ‘ -._ I III iIV II‘ - in I Drain- If I ._ ._ I Ii I‘ .‘II LI. ' . Unpublished manuscript, Oregon Social Learning Center, Eugene, Oregon. Holleran, P. A., Littman, D. C, Freund, R. D., & Schmaling, K. B. (l982b). A signal detection approach to social perception: Identification of negative and positive behaviors by parents of normal and problem children. MW Psyohology, _9, 547-557. Johnson, S.M., & Lobitz, GK. (1974). Parental manipulation of child behavior in home observations. Joomfl of Aooligz! Bohavior Aoalysig, 2, 23-31. Johnson, S.M., Wahl, G., Martin, S., & Johansson, S. (1973). How deviant is the normal child? A behavioral analysis of the preschool child and his family. In R. Rubin, J. Brady, & J. Henerson (Eds.), Advaogo in bohovior thorooy; Vol, 4 (pp. 37-54). New York: Academic Press. Kaufman, J ., & Zigler, E. (1987). Do abused children become abusive parents? Amorim Journal of Qflhoosychiam, 2, 186-192. Kazdin, A. (1982). Symptom substitution, generalization, and response covariation: Implications for psychotherapy outcome. Psychologigol Bolleoo' , fl, 349-365. Knutson, J .F. (1982). Perspective on chapter 7. In G. Patterson, goeroivo famfl y process (pp.l65-l66). Eugene, Oregon: Castilia Press. 260 Lobitz, G.K., & Johnson, S.M. (1975a). Parental manipulation of the behavior of normal and deviant children. Child Dovoloomont, go, 719-726. Lobitz, G. K. & Johnson, S. M. (1975b). Normal versus deviant children: A multi-method comparison WW 3 353- 374. Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. (1985a). The utility of differentiating between mixed and pure forms of antisocial child behavior. f A ' P l , 13, 315-336. Loeber, R, & Schmaling, K. B. (1985b) Empirical evidence for overt and covert patterns of antisocial conduct problems: A meta-analysis. MW Embology ll 337- 252 Lorber, R. (1981). Parental tracking of childhood behavior as a function of family stress (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1981). Wm Lutemational, 51.2.(5), 2066-13. Main, M. & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting rejection of her infant from mother’s representation of her own experience: Implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle. Child Aoooeg Q, Nogloot, 8, 203-217. Main, M. & Goldwyn, R. (1985). MW Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley. Main, M. & Goldwyn, R. (1989). Adolt agachmon; gm 2 god clgooifioao'on §y§gm. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research, Monoggoho of the Sooiety for Regroh in Child gvoloomont, S_Q (1-2 Serial No. 209), 66-104. Main, M., & Weston, D. (1982). Avoidance of the attachment figure in infancy: Descriptions and interpretations. In C.M. Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Ibo olaoo of agaoflont in human behavior. New York: Basic Books. Minuchin, S. (1974). Familios and Family Thomy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Myers, J. (1979). Fundamengls of Exmrimental Design (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 261 Morris, D. (1981). Attachment and intimacy. In G. Stricker (Ed.), My (pp.305- 323). New York: Plenum. O’Leary, K.D. , & Emery, RE. (1984). Marital discord and child behavior problems. In M. Levine & P. Satz (Eds.), WW (pp- 345-364). Baltimore: University Park Press. Olweus, D. (1980). Familial and temperamental determinants of aggressive behavior in adolescents: A causal analysis. v 1 m n P h l , 16(6), 644-660. Patterson, G. R. (1975). Multiple evaluations of a parent-training program. In T. Thompson & W Dookens (EdS) W (pp 299- 322). New York: Academic Press. Patterson, G. R. (1976). The aggressive child: Victim and architect of a coercive system. In E]. Mash, L..A Hamerlynck, & L..C Handy (Eds. ), Bgohaflor MW. New York. Brunner/Mazel. Patterson, G. R. (1982), W. Eugene, Oregon: Castilia Press. Patterson, G.R. (1986a). Performance models for antisocial boys. Amofloao Poyohologist, 4_1, 432-444. Patterson, G. R. (1986b). Maternal rejection: Determinant or product for deviant child behavior? In W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), WWW (pp. 73-94). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Patterson, G.R. (1986c). The contribution of siblings to training for fighting: A microsocial analysis. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), D'VlmtI‘II‘, If ‘nIIII. .. I ’II II 1 Be ViIr' R .'.._ I Thri , I I ‘ (pp. 235-261). Orlando, Florida: Academic Press. Patterson, G.R., & Dishion, T.J. (1985). Contributions of families and peers to delinquency. Criminology, 23, 63-79. Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Bank, L. (1984). Family interaction: A process model of deviancy training. W, _Q, 253-267. Patterson, G.R., & Reid, LB. (1973). Intervention for families of aggressive boys: A replication study. Behovior Research and Therapy, Ll, 383-394. Patterson, G.,R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1984). The correlation of family management practices and delinquency. Child Devolopmont, 55, 1299-1307. 262 Quay, H. (1979) Classification. In H.C. Quay & J..S Werry (Eds.), W (2nd ed”) New York: Wiley Quay, H. (1986a). Classification. In H..C Quay & J..S Werry (Eds.), W (3rd ed”) New York: Wiley Quay, H. (1986b). Conduct disorders. In H. C. Quay & J. S. Werry (Eds. ), P l i i r f hil h (3rd ed..) New York: Wiley. Rickard, K.M., Forehand, R., Wells, K.C., Griest, D.L., & McMahon, R.J. (1981). Factors in the referral of children for behavioral treatment: A comparison of mothers of clinic-referred deviant, clinic-referred non-deviant and non-clinic children. MW, 12. 201-205- Rickard, K.M., Graziano, W., & Forehand, R. (1984). Parental expectations and childhood deviance in clinic-referred and non-clinic children. M mam. 11(2). 179-186. Ricks, M. H. (1985). The social transmission of parental behavior: Attachment across generations. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds. ), Growing points in attachment theory and research W 50 (1-2 Serial No. 209), 211-227. Robinson, EA. (1985). Coercion theory revisited: Toward a new theoretical perspective on the etiology of conduct disorders. W, 5, 597- 625. Robinson, E. A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1987). Social learning theory and family psychopathology: A Kantian model in behaviorism? In T. Jacob (Ed.,) Family mtegacg'gn nags! psyg hgpathglggy; Thggrias, mgmgig, Q51 findings (pp. 117-162). New York: Plenum Press. Russo, D. ,Ca,taldo M. ,& Cushing, P. (1981). Compliance training and behavioral covariation in the treatment of multiple behavior problems. Wed Behavigr Agflygi 15, 1_4(3), 209- 222. Rutter, M, & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental psychopathology. In P Mussen (Ed. ), I. . ”Ck 9f hil. 3 h. 10‘ V01 4 9; «.-_.':!IflI11'I 1- K 1 Q .A -1. Wm (pp. 776-911). New York: Wiley and Sons. Rutter, M., & Giller, H (1983). Juvenile delingggngy; Trend§ aad pefipegg'veg England: Penguin. 263 Sallows, GO. (1973). Responsiveness of deviant and normal children to naturally occurring consequences (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1972). W 3302), 6092-3. Schmaling, K. B. (1983). T hni r n n r ° n f hil v' . Unpublished manuscript, Oregon Social Learning Center, Eugene, Oregon. Schmaling, K. B., & Patterson, GR. (1984, November). W I‘ .. «. I I I .1,__ hild I‘thrgII Ir'a {In I ‘ hilI in e Im. Paper presented at American Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Philadelphia. Shaver, P. (1984). Attributions and child-caretaker interactions: Commentary by PhilipShaver. h f i er ° ' v mn,52(l, Serial No. 205), 71-76. Shaw, DA. (1972). Family maintenance schedules for deviant behavior (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon. 1971). WWW. 32(9). 5458-B. Simon, R.L., Whitbeck, L.B., Conger, R.D., & Chyi-In, W. (1991). Intergenerational transmission of harsh parenting. v 1 n P h l , 22, 59- 69. Snyder, J .J . (1977). A reinforcement analysis of interaction in problem and non- problem children. Jddmal of Abndrmfl Esydhdldgy, _8_6, 528-535. Snyder, J.J . & Patterson, GR. (1987). Family interaction and delinquent behavior. In H. Quay (Ed.), fiadbdok of jdvgnile delingdangy (pp. 216-243). New York: Wiley & Sons. Sroufe, L.A. , & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction of relationships. In W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Rl ' 11 hi v 1 n (pp. 51-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Thompson. R.J., & Bernal, ME. (1982). Factors associated with parent labeling of children referred for conduct problems. 152qu df Abndrmal Child Psyghdlogy, IQ, 191-202. Wahl, G., Johnson, 8., Johansson, 8., & Martin, S. (1974). An operant analysis of child-family interaction. Behavior Thgmy, 5, 64-78. Wahler, R.G. (1969). Oppositional children: A quest for parental reinforcement control. Joumg df Appligd Behavidr Analysis, 2, 159-170. 264 Wahler, R.G. (1980). The insular mother: Her problems in parent-child treatment. WWW, L3. 207-219. Wahler, R.G. (1982) Perspective on chapter 4. In G. Patterson, mm was (pp. 82-83). Eugene, Oregon: Castilia Press. Wahler, R.G., & Afton, AD. (1980). Attentional processes in insular and noninsular mothers: Some differences in their summary reports about child problem behaviors. End d Bahavidr lihggapy, 2, 25-41. Wahler, W.G., & Dumas, J.E. (1986). "A chip off the old block": Some interpersonal characteristics of coercive children across generations. In P. Strain, M. Guralnick, & H. Walker (Eds.), hil r n’s ' B h v' r v1 11 Wu (pp. 49-91). Orlando: Academic Press. Wahler, W. G. & Dumas, J. E. (1987). Family factors in childhood psychology: Toward a coercion-neglect model. In T. Jacob (Ed.),Emily_in_tg_1'a9_d9__aad MW: (pp 581-627) New York: Plenum Press.