m «1“ ..- - ‘¥—I in: ' .M q‘urW; ‘ .4:- .1 'M-leml 37;” ~19 ,. y“ {[11 ‘0‘ d 2 ‘ .w up M MW\M M . Muwm w ~ 3 3‘3 ‘3‘ MM: MM}; 3'! 31* MM“ ' ""‘MM'M‘MM 3...; ..~ M *LMMM“33“‘M.~3M““5343 ”MM ‘ 3333 F3333 ‘3‘! 3“. 3‘1 M3332“ M33"; ll 1“ M . ["3 M: m 31334133333 ”3333‘ ' ' '3 ‘1' MuhfifMirwmeMM“”M%@%M . M . z M ’3‘“ .MMMM'; MM ‘2‘ M 3 TM" 3 3313333333334332M311311333135; ‘F‘ 23‘ ‘ ME ‘6‘? 3' '" . .‘ ‘33'1‘33‘513‘133‘3“.u!“‘“““M “5 :‘ M' 5 “‘3‘ "‘ I} 33333.“: 1‘3“!“ 3" .“31‘3‘3‘ - I ' - .. “3"; x14. ,' {7339232333321 8311!! 1;: {31in fig; ‘. .2:- 00—... , -::41::.'... . ' ”do-:- 1 000‘ M m E: “a m..- - M' I'.‘ 5 x; f "u . ~' E“ ' z - 3 . .. I}E:_M.;€q“§£t.- ' v‘ . 13"‘5‘33335 is '1" 3 [1‘1“ "3 "‘3 "{3}“ ‘ it Jig. 'uv< v... . O 6 N... Lfifg’; “:‘rtfif-M - . My ‘4. . . |.I .4. a a? a 5,.- i}; Han" w: {#51 {‘1} . . v ,4 can r- 3." .4 5..-..." ’ 1' .‘ nor-ca u --o- u ’4’ ,- .wo- -...... .. . m .o.a - ‘~ fl ' Lo.v—ao- . o . ov l"l- -)-‘a1 v— A N“; .‘ :.;}M;,..':té-, \ ‘. - .h M' : M "M . I“ ' 31““ "In "t. W“ :M‘ ‘3‘ I"M I . . 4":1': *" "“3”“ . _..~ ‘91: ‘33?! h- 53:!“1..g,‘1u :. ' . 3 film! “3.431 I :3‘ fit? ‘éifg' .th M.“ [N ;: ..."‘ gird-r; -. .5 ‘ ' :1... .113 | In Tu. l M Mm, zi l w ‘ ; 5 ‘9; ’i‘r‘ M! {13:13 3‘1"? 333,131} ‘33 ‘& M193“; ‘ *H .- 32:1? :1“ 3 7? ”f”. "‘ 3‘3 M 13;“ M M‘s aw? ., «'3‘ :3; a": M “‘M 1““ 3"13‘33‘3‘1‘13“: ‘lvaii 1‘31“ 133.3“ ‘3‘“ “"333 PM if!“ 'I‘W’H‘MMqW'“ 'MM 3 “3.3M“!33313;,Mizligmggg ‘ H“M'MUM1uM‘hwgdhg.‘Hymnfi’ MMHMMJHN “.331533- 13-33 .333 331;. 3333 1133.333! 3 ‘3 {5‘3“ {M3333 3‘3“ {‘3‘ {33“}‘3‘333 smug} «bun.» 1Mh‘3‘1i_ M. ,2! 3 ‘3‘: .3313}: ““H !m3if‘ir‘W‘iiifidfl3‘33;3331- 1.333323 3311 MM 3313;.“ ...; WMMMMMM alt:g3l3333gM-uMJM l , ’33. 11}3£13‘f1‘3"1d.}t‘:‘. ' . ”(3111‘ #41“va | xi, ‘3 :. mm.“n‘"‘r"1' MN MflWMMM3MwWWWMWMWW3“M 1333 Ill '\ \ irimmmmnmi This is to certify that the dissertation entitled PERCEPTIONS OF CRIME AND CONFLICT IN URBAN PARKS AND FORESTS presented by THERESA N. NESTOVER has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D. degree in Geography ,1 $74 W4kaf22§1 a jor pro‘essor Mic/:92. Date MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 _____———__-‘. _ __ _ _ . _ PLACE N RETURN BOX to roman thb checkout from your mood. TO AVOID FINES Mum on or Moro dot. duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE .55? 2 5 1295 MSU I: An Affimmivo Adlai/Equal Opportunity Induction WMMHJ /5/<7-/7/7 Pa PERCEPTIONS OF CRIME AND CONFLICT IN URBAN PARKS AND FORESTS By Theresa N. Westover A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Geography 1982 ABSTRACT PERCEPTIONS OF CRIME AND CONFLICT IN URBAN PARKS AND FORESTS by Theresa N. Westover Research related to environmental perception has revealed the importance of the "images" of places in shaping interactions between people and their environment. Recently, park settings have been in- vestigated to determine their "images," as well as the kinds of behavior, activities, and social atmosphere people expect to encounter. Because one aspect of park environments of special concern to park agency personnel is antisocial behavior, park users' perceptual and behavioral responses to antisocial behavior in district level parks are examined in this study. Face-to-face structured interviews of randomly selected park visitors were conducted in three Midwestern district level parks. The relationships between respondents' perceptions of personal safety in the park and their individual characteristics and reported behavior are examined with the use of simple proportions and nonparametric measures of association, statistical significance, and proportional reduction in error. Study results indicate that district level parks are generally perceived to be safe environments. Evenings are considered less safe than days in the parks. Only gender is strongly associated with both perceptions of safety and reported avoidance behavior. Female respondents are much more likely than males to report feeling unsafe in the park and to report avoidance behavior due to concerns about safety. Mbre generally, there is also a fairly strong and significant association between perceptions of park safety and reported avoidance behavior. Study results have implications both for the park manager or planner wishing to improve recreation provision for his or her clien- tele and for research addressing spatial decision-making, behavior, and perceptions of public spaces. These data also indicate that perceptions of safety directly influence behavior in the park. Confirmation of this relationship, however, requires detailed observation of park users' on-site behavior. Dedicated to Ron Hodgson; my first and favorite mentor, for his belief in my ability and his persistence in convincing me to try. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS It is both traditional and appropriate to begin acknowledgements by thanking the members of the dissertation committee. I feel a par- ticular debt of gratitude to my committee, Gary Manson, Joe T. Darden, Joseph Fridgen, and Robert Wittick, for their guidance and assistance during the dissertation process and for the many lessons they have taught me about research, writing, and academic excellence. Special thanks go to my major advisor, Gary Manson, for his patience, concientious attention, and many valuable contributions to all phases of my academic development at Michigan State University. These few pages of acknowledgement are the only ones in this manuscript that have not had the benefit of his careful, word-by-word review. Dan Stynes and Bruce Pigozzi also deserve a special vote of appreciation for their many hours of help and encouragement during the research process and for their patient guidance through the thickets of statistical methods and analytical thinking. Primary financial support for this research was provided by a grant from the Urban Forest Recreation Research Project, North Central Forest Experiment Station in Chicago, Illinois. Additional funds were provided by the Pacific Northwest Forest Experiment Station in Seattle, Washington. In addition to their assistance in providing financial support, personal thanks are much deserved by John Dwyer, Project iii Leader in Chicago, and Chris Christensen, Research Scientist, in Seattle for their deeply appreciated and frequently sought advice, encouragement, and interest throughout the research process. Thanks also go to Mike Chubb for his role in grant administration and data collection. The data collection process involved the assistance and cooper- ation of many people. Bob Callecod and his research staff in the Hennepin County Parszeserve District devoted many hours and much care- ful attention to collecting data in Coon Rapids Dam and Elm Creek Park Reserves. Their assistance and cooperation in the project were crucial to its completion and their patience and help are very much appreciated. Data collection would not have been possible in Potter Park without the cooperation of Lansing Park and Recreation Department Director, Doug Finley, and Potter Park Manager Gerald Miller and his staff. Many thanks are also due the research staff who helped collect data in Potter Park -- Cindy Studer, Lori Indenbaum, Wrick Dunning, and Joe Thome. Their cheerfulness, energy, and care in the face of frequently tedious data collection and coding tasks were always appreciated. Of even greater personal value, however, are the bonds of friendship and affection that were built among us during that long, hot summer in the park. Less clearly defined, but equally valuable, are the contributions of friends and family during the last several years. Thanks must go, first, to my family in California for their patience, support and encouragement during our long separation. I am sorry for the many times I should have been there to share their joys and sorrows and was not. iv Heartfelt thanks also are due to Joe and Cynthia Fridgen and Bonnie, Dan, and Colin Stynes for providing a much needed sense of "family" in Michigan. Special thanks go to Bonnie Stynes, who always found the time to share both the discouraging and anxious times as well as the joys of the past few years. The only regrets I have in finally com- pleting this phase of my academic career are the miles now separating me from my East Lansing "family" and those that continue to separate me from my California home. Last, but far from least, I wish to acknowledge my endless gratitude to my husband, Joe Thome. He has been deeply involved in all phases of the dissertation.' He collected data, helped type early drafts, and spent many hours drafting maps, including the final copies in this manuscript. While it was this research project that initially brought us together, Joe's friendship, encouragement, and support were instrumental in its final completion. Thanks, Joe, for being a partner in every sense of the word. TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION . The Geographic Context: Identifying an Investigative Framework . Perceptions of Park Safety and Behavioral Response . CHAPTER TWO: LITERATURE REVIEW . Introduction . Parks as Behavior Settings . Perception and Behavior in Recreation Settings . Summary . Crime and Conflict in Park Settings . Geography of Crime and Perceptions of Crime . Identifying and Measuring Depreciative Behavior in Parks . Crime and Conflict in Rural Parks: Probelms and Perceptions . Crime and Conflict in Urban and Suburban Parks: Problems and Perceptions . Behavioral Response to Park Crime and Conflict . Summary . Defining the Investigative Questions . CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY . Study Park Settings . Park User Survey . vi 11 11 13 19 . 24 . 26 . 26 28 . 39 44 SO . 53 S4 57 . S7 67 CHAPTER FOUR: DATA ANALYSIS Relative Park Safety . Association of Park Users' Individual Characteristics with Safety Related Perceptions and Behavior . Age, Gender, Familiarity, and Perceptions of Safety . Perceived Social Homogeneity and Perceptions of Safety . Experiences with Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Perceptions of Safety . ' Safety Perceptions and Avoidance Behavior . Personal Characteristics and Avoidance Behavior . Experiences with Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Reported Avoidance . Reasons for Avoidance Behavior . Summary of Acceptance or Rejection of Study Hypotheses . CHAPTER FIVE: SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . Summary and Discussion of Results . Relative Park Safety . Association of Park Users' Individual Characteristics . 111 with Safety Related Perceptions and Behavior . Association of Exposure to Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Safety Related Perceptions and Behavior . . . . . . . . . Association of Perceptions of Safety with Avoidance . Study Limitations . Future Research . Theoretical Implications . Implications and Recommendations for Park.Planning and Management . . . . . . . . . . REFERENCES . vii 72 72 78 78 82 . 84 . 90 . 96 96 . 104 106 109 109 . 109 . 113 116 117 119 121 . 127 133 LIST OF TABLES Physical and Managerial Aspects of Park Settings: The Recreation Opportunity Spectrum . . The Perceptual and Behavioral Environments of‘ Park Settings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Demographic Characteristics of Park User Survey Sample . Measures of Interviewer Bias: Park User Survey . Park Safety During Day and Evening . Relative Park Safety . . . . . Fear Index Scores . Association of Respondents' Personal Factors with Fear Index Scores . Age and Gender of Respondents Who Report Feeling Unsafe in the Parks . Residential Proximity and Park Visit Frequency of Respondents Who Report Feeling Unsafe Alone in the Parks . The Association of Perceptions of Social Homogeneity with Perceptions of Park Safety . Association of Witnessing Antisocial Behavior in the Park with Perceptions of Safety . The Experience of Respondents Who Report Feeling Unsafe Alone in the Park with Crime, Alcohol Consumption, and Drugs in the Parks. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .10 Association of Age with Witnessing Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Frequency of Park Visitation.. .11 Frequency of Experiencing Serious Crime, User Behavior Problems, and Park Visitation for Each Age Category . viii . 17 . 25 69 7O 73 74 . 77 . 8O 81 83 85 87 . 88 89 . 91 .12 .13 .14 .15 .16 .17 .18 .19 .20 .21 Association of Perceptions of Safety and Avoidance Behavior . Proportion of Respondents Who Report Avoidance Behavior and Their Fear Index Scores . Association of Safety Related Perceptions and Behavior and Reported Evening Visitation . . . . . . . . . . . Association of Respondents' Personal Characteristics and Perceptions of Park Social Homogeneity with Avoidance Behavior . PrOportion of Male and Female Respondents Reporting Avoidance Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Association of Witnessing Antisocial Behavior with, Reported Avoidance Behavior . The Experience of Respondents Reporting Avoidance Behavior with Crime, Litter, Vandalism, Rowdy Behavior and Traffic Violations in the Park . Reports of Antisocial Behavior WitneSsed in the Parks . Reasons for Avoiding the Park or Park Areas . Who Causes Problems in the Park? . ix . 92 94 9S . 97 . 98 99 .101 . 103 . 105 107 1.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 LIST OF FIGURES Sonnenfeld's Behavioral Classification of Environment . Potter Park . Elm Creek Park Reserve . Elm Creek Study Area . Coon Rapids Dam Regional Park. 60 62 64 65 CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION An important goal of research in human geography is to better understand how people interact with the environment. Pattison identifies this as the "man-land tradition" in geography [1]. This quest for understanding has led geographers into many different avenues of inquiry.' Some have concentrated on people-environment interactions in particular types of settings such as urban locales, workplaces or recreation sites. Others have chosen to pursue insights into the spatial pattern, causes, and effects of particular behaviors such as migration, home or recreation location choice, crime, innovation diffusion, or human response to various hazards [2,3,4,S,6,7]. Many geographic inquiries into human-environment interaction are pursued within the conceptual framework of "environmental perception." This approach stresses the importance of peoples' "images of . surroundings as a key to unraveling the nature of mansland transactions" [2,p.ii]. Thus, the social meanings that people attribute to different components of the environmental setting shape their behavioral responses to and within that setting. The present study of fear of crime in urban and suburban district level parks focuses on perceptions of particular behaviors within a particular setting. While organized within the conceptual framework of environmental perception, this study also draws from and contributes to the subfields of recreation geography and the geography of crime, as well as the interdisciplinary field of leisure study. Crime and conflict, ranging from controversies over nude sunbathing to violent crimes, are increasingly pervasive and costly problems in recreation settings [8,9,10,11,12,13]. Surveys of recreation agency personnel repeatedly reveal vandalism, litter, and other user problems to be high priority management concerns [9,14,15]. The National Park Service reports a 7.2 percent increase in felony crimes between 1975 and 1978 and estimates Operating costs due to vandalism have more than doubled between 1974 and 1978 [16]. Some urban area park managers report vandalism costs as high as $1000 a week [17]. In addition to the direct costs of increased maintenance and enforcement, many indirect costs are also incurred. Recreation opportunity is decreased through restrictions and reductions in facilities and programs. Users, particularly in urban areas, may modify their visitation patterns or stay away entirely due to fear of conflict or criminal attack [18]. Overall, antisocial behavior problems, together with the anxieties and expecta- tions they produce, serve to decrease the satisfaction of both park users and employees. This study examines the extent to which selected park settings are considered threatening or conflict laden by park users. In addition, the behavioral responses of park users to both perceived and observable characteristics of selected park environments are identified. The specific research questions examined are: (1) Do park users feel safe in the parks? (2) How do the individual characteristics of park users relate to their perceptions of park safety and behavioral responses to perceived safety? (3) How do park users' perceptions of park safety relate to their behavior in the park? The Geggraphic Context: Identifying_an Investigative Framework Since classical times, human interaction with the environment has been a prominent topic in geographic inquiry [1]. Within this tradition, cultural geographers have traced human actions and societal development through the study of artifacts, settlement patterns, and other observable phenomena [2]. Social and urban geographers have approached the question of human-environment interaction through the study of aggregate behavior and the spatial eXpression of social institutions. Work in urban ecology has searched for correlates of social phenomena such as juvenile delinquency in the structural and social contexts of cities [4,7,19]. It has been argued that traditional approaches to people- environment relationships have ignored the importance of individual roles, decision, relationships, attitudes, and behaviors that combine to form the observable aggregate patterns [20]. This desire to focus on individuals rather than aggregates has prompted work in environmental perception and the emerging subfield of behavioral geography. Lowenthal's "Geography, Experience, and Imagination," published in 1961, is generally credited as the major impetus for the development of environ- mental perception research in geography [21]. In this article and in subsequent works, Lowenthal points out the importance of cultural and personal values in perceptions and uSes of the physical environment [21,22,23,24]. Cultural and social geographers embraced this viewpoint as consistent with their basic concepts and investigative techniques. Over the last twenty years geographers' interests in the "image" or perceptions of different human settings have stimulated work in a variety of directions. Some have approached questions of cultural or shared "images" of places from the historical perspective by examining the influence of perceptions on past population movements and settlement patterns [25,26]. Others have investigated the influence of these images on current residential preference patterns, landscape assessments, public policy fbrmulation, individual decision-making and overt behavior [27,28,29,30,31,32,33]. Still others have concentrated on how individuals deveIOp images or "mental maps" of areas and how spatial infbrmation is learned, used, and remembered [34,35,36,37]. Much of the work in environmental perception, particularly that on spatial learning and cognitive maps, is based on the assumption that the process and product of perception can be decomposed and that the component parts may be measured and compared to an objective reality. These assumptions are consistent with a logical-positivist philosophy which assumes that human behavior is ultimately measurable and is, in some instances, amenable to mathematical modelling [33,36]. Concurrent and often intertwined with these works are geographical studies of perception and behavior more characteristic of traditional cultural geography and humanist philosophies, stressing more holistic and subjective approaches to understanding the cultural landscape and its meaning [2,38,39,40,41,42]. Questions regarding the inherent "measurability" of environmental images and their influence on individual or collective behavior have been raised. Bunting and Guelke contend that, due largely to methodological problems, work in environmental perception and- behavioral geography has contributed little of explanatory value in understanding spatial behavior [43]. Their objections are based on the recognized difficulties of eliciting reliable information from individuals regarding such subtle and complex psychological constructs as environmen- tal ”images" and the lack of a standardized, rigorously tested methodology for doing so. Further, they protest that the alleged link between these images and observable behavior is based on very little empirical evidence. Their proposed avenue for the subfield's return to relevance is a reorientation to describing and evaluating observable, overt behaviors. The environmental images that people hold and the influence of those images on behavior can, they argue, be better identified by inference from actual behavior. Further, by focusing on overt behavior of geographic significance one commences one's investigation on a foundation of solid, verifiable fact. Even if one fails to provide an acceptable explanation, at least a good description of a specific group of pertinent value to other research could be provided [43, p.462]. In response, researchers in this area agree that there are methodological weaknesses in current approaches but argue that Bunting And Guelke fail to recognize the relative youth of the field, the advances that have been made, and the complexity of the problem. Shifting the focus to regard only observable behavioral responses to environments is retrogressive, not progressive, in their view. Further, they argue, there is ample empirical evidence to suggest a direct link between environmental perceptions and behaVior [44,45,46]. Besides, Saarinen points out, if Bunting and Guelke really believe there is no demon- strable link between perceived images and observed behavior, then their criticisms of measurement accuracy and methodology are "beside the point." Saarinen states, if images are important we should find some way of measuring them. Even a rough measure of a significant variable is better than a precise measure of something that does not matter. Along with this we should make every effort to understand how much and in what directions disparities between image and action probably will go [4S,p.465]. One way to avoid some of the difficulties alluded to above is to work within a framework that clearly identifies the various aspects of environmental perception and behavior to be examined. Responding to the confusion arising from vague definitions of "environment" as used in behavioral and social science, Sonnenfeld has proposed a four level, hierarchial classification of the human environment [47]. The first and most general level is the geographical environment which includes all objects and phenomena external to the individual, whether or not he or she is aware of them (Figure 1). Next is the operational environment, defined as that portion of the geographical environment which impinges on the individual and influences behavior in one way or another. Operational environments, unlike the geographical environment, differ from group to group and individual to individual. Within the operational environment each individual possesses unique perceptual and behavioral environments. An individual's perceptual environment is comprised of those environmental factors that he or she is conscious of. This includes both physiological stimuli such as color and light intensity, and the meanings associated with these stimuli, through learning and experience. Thus, the perceptual environ- ment has "both sensory and symbolic dimensions" [47]. It includes various environmental cues that have meaning only when interpreted through cultural and individual value systems and eXperiences. The last GEOGRAPHICAL OPERATIONAL PERCEPTUAL BEHAVIORAL SONNENFELD'S BEHAVIORAL CLASSIFICATION OF ENVIRONMENT FIGURE I level in the hierarchy is the behavioral environment or that portion of the perceptual environment which elicits overt behavior. This active interaction with the environment may take the form of modifying either some aspect of the environment (change) or one's exposure to it (movement). This classification and the distinction between behavioral and perceptual environments allows a conscious differentiation in research design and analysis between, on the one hand, what peOple are aware of, how they feel about the environment, and their responses to hypothetical situations, i.e. aspects of their perceptual environments, and, on the other hand, how people behave within the environment and overtly respond to various environmental stimuli, i.e. elements of their behavioral environments. These distinctions facilitate investigation of the relationships between the perceptual and behavioral environments. When applied to a particular setting or type of environment, Sonnenfeld's approach is augmented by perspectives from ecological psychology. Ecological psychologists Barker and Wright set out in the late 19405 to examine human behavior in natural, rather than laboratory, settings. During their field research they identified "behavior settings" which provide the milieu and environmental cues eliciting generalizable or "standing" patterns of behavior [27, pp.84-88]. Thus, they established that people identify certain behaviors with certain environments. Following this paradigm, researchers investigate how the available settings for behavior (1) are interpreted by the people in them, and (2) facilitate appropriate behavior patterns. Behavior settings include both physical, or design elements and social characteristics. Research investigating the influence of design on behavior ranges from micro-level studies of room geography to investigations of response to architectural elements of cities and neighborhoods [27,48,49,50,Sl]. Introduction of the social aspects of behavior settings adds considerable complexity to the investigation but it also enriches the eXplanations of observed behavior. Wicker suggests that behavior settings have self- regulating mechanisms that tend to support acceptable standing patterns of behavior [52, pp.62-64]. Thus, people either work out mutually satisfactory behavior patterns within a given behavior setting or one is rejected by the other, i.e. the potential participant leaves. Much of the work in ecological psychology focuses on the behavioral environment, but the importance of the perceptual environment is recognized. Clearly, interpretations of environmental cues or symbols are important in determining subsequent behavior and these interpretations depend on individual personal and contextual elements [51,52,53]. Furthermore, investigation of how people feel about environments, what their emotional response or satisfaction level is, remains a viable strand of the pe0p1e-environment and humanist traditions within geography. Perceptions of Park Safety and Behavioral Response The operational environment of park users is defined as the observable physical park characteristics, e.g. park design, facilities, and setting; social characteristics, e.g. types of users, their behavior, and spatial distribution in the park; and managerial characé teristics, e.g. the presence of enforcement officers, the park's hours of operation, and entry controls. Park users' perceptual environments consist of their interpretation of these overt characteristics. The 10 behavioral environments of park users are demonstrated in both observable patterns of behavior in the park and users' reports of what they do and where they go while visiting the park. By examining each of these levels of environment, as they relate to perceptions of park safety, intercon- nections between the different levels may be illuminated. Further, the "behavior settings" provided by district level urbanand suburban parks and various responses to them can be explored in a limited fashion. This study addresses, first, the overall "image" of the parks as safe or unsafe environments. Both the perceived safety of the park relative to other settings and temporal variations in perceived safety are examined. Next, the influence of users' individual characteristics, such as gender, age, race, and familiarity with the park, on safety perceptions and related behavior is evaluated and the direct association between perceptions of safety and behavioral response is addressed. Investigation of safety related perceptions and behavior of park users has both pragmatic and theoretical significance. Park planners, management, and enforcement personnel need such information to identify problem areas and design more effective crime prevention strategies. Further, theory development in both leisure research and behavioral geography requires insights into how pe0ple perceive particular social and physical environments and how these perceptions are translated into overt behavior. CHAPTER TWO: LITERATURE REVIEW Introduction The subfield of recreation geography includes a variety of research approaches and topics. Geographic analyses of recreation phenomena began to appear during the 19305 with discussions of rec- reational land use and tourism [54]. Through the 19605 recreation geographers continued to focus on the economic impacts and signifi- cance of recreation related development [54,55]. Stankey notes that recreation geography has traditionally been considered a subcomponent of economic geography by virtue of this early emphasis on land use and economic aspects of recreation resources and activities [54,p.76]. After World War II recreation activities, travel, and both pub- lic and private recreation related expenditures expanded dramatically [S6,pp.34-39]. Correspondingly, recreation related topics received increased academic attention from geographers and other social scien- tists. By the early 19705 recreation geography textbooks began to appear and geographers had assumed leading roles in both government agency and interdisciplinary academic investigations of recreation topics [S4,pp.78-79]. Many of these studies continued to address inherently spatial issues of recreation related travel and site choice. Mathematical models predicting recreation travel flows and recreation 11 12 facility use levels were developed and continue to be an important component in the increasingly diverse subfield of recreation geography. It became apparent, however, that the spatial considerations of location, distance, travel time, and p0pulation density did not fully explain observable variations in recreation site choice or use levels. Geographers and other recreation researchers began to address more sub- jective issues such as attitudes, landscape preferences, and recreation facility development preferences in their efforts to understand and predict recreation behavior [59,60,61]. At the same time, geographers and others working from humanist or environmental perception viewpoints began to discuss the meaning or "image" of recreation pursuits and places over time and space [62,63,64]. Tuan and Graber, for example, demonstrate how the symbolic value, use, and cultural importance of wilderness areas have changed over time [65,66,67]. Thus, recreation geography has eXpanded from an early preoccupation with economic is— sues to encompassaifuller range of geographic perspectives and approaches. Geographic discussions of place ”images" are consistent with work by sociologists, anthr0pologi5ts, and psychologists which address the social or symbolic meanings associated with different kinds of environmental settings [2,3,21,39,49,52,53,65,68,69,70]. The various components that combine to define place "images" or "social defini- tions," and their significance in influencing behavior remains a new and growing field of academic interest. This perspective offers Opportunities for significant insights into recreation places and behavior than can enrich the subfield of recreation geography. 13 Images of recreation environments and behavior in park settings have been examined at several scales of analysis using a variety of approaches. Recently, theoretical structures relating recreation behavior to particular types of park settings have begun to emerge. In this chapter these classification schemes and definitional structures are examined, together with supporting evidence from studies in various settings. Each of Sonnenfeld's levels of environment, i.e. the opera- tional, perceptual, and behavioral, is examined in terms of how they ’differ in different types of park settings. Next, the specific problems of crime and conflict in recreation environments are addressed. Again following Sonnenfeld's structure, objective evidence regarding the prevalence of depreciative behavior in parks is first discussed, followed by examination of available materials illustrating park users' perceptual and behavioral responses to these problems. These previous studies of depreciative behavior in different park settings form the basis for the present study. The specific research questions addressed and hypotheses tested are presented in the final section of this chapter. Parks as Behavior Settings Public outdoor recreation settings carry various administrative labels such as "park," "forest," "forest preserve," "park reserve," "arboretum," "recreation area," and so forth. For purposes of this discussion the term "park" is generic, including all types of outdoor recreation settings. Similarly, although there has been endless academic debate regarding the difference between "recreation” and "leisure," these terms will be used interchangeably in reference to the 14 types of activities that typically occur in park settings [56,70]. Parks may be classified according to level of administrative responsibility, size, location, or any number of other criteria. Several researchers have proposed continuums or hierarchial arrangements for classifying or describing different types of parks. Helburn proposes that parks may be arranged along a "wildness continuum" ranging from the largely artificial, often small, highly deve10ped and heavily used parks typical of urban areas to the large, remote, undeveloped, "wilderness" parks in more rural locations where use is more dispersed [64]. Intrinsic to this continuum of "naturalness" is the influence of distance and accessibility. A5 distance from concentrations of human activities and habitations increases, so does the expression of "natural" processes or wildness. While pockets of "nature" exist in the form of gardens, parks, and vacant lots even in the midst of the metropolis, they cannot maintain the ecological diversity or natural balance possible in more extensive, rural locations. Further, it is largely the very remoteness and inaccessibility of existing "wilderness" areas that have preserved them in a "natural" or "wild" state. Helburn also suggests that people seek to increase their contact with nature and tend to move along the continuum toward a more "wild" setting when choosing an outdoor recreation location. He exemplifies the incremental or gradational character of this movement by noting that an inner city resident will find the landscape and animals of an agricultural scene quite as exotic and interesting as the suburban dweller might regard a national or state park with herds of deer and an occasional coyote. Other researchers have noted pervasive preferences for more natural elements in all types of settings and suggest this may be referable to biological or 15 genetic "programming" established through the evolutionary process [72]. If the desire for contact with nature is an incremental one, then it is reasonable to expect that the numbers and diversity of recreation area users will decrease as areas become more "wild," more remote and inac- cessible. Lee defines a hierarchy of neighborhood, district, regional, and remote outdoor places whiCh is also based on ideas of accessibility or location and users' expectations [73]. He characterizes "neighborhood" parks as set in residential areas with primarily local users who consider the park part of their evertday space to be governed by local social norms. "District" and "regional" outdoor places attract users from more than one neighborhood by drawing people from a wider surrounding area, and are defined by non-local visitors in terms of more formalized "rules of conduct" associated with the specific attractions present at the site (museums, picnic grounds, nature areas, and so forth). However, these "district" and "regional" parks may also be used by local residents who view and treat the site as part of their everyday territory to be governed by local groups' activities and definitions. Finally, remote recreation areas draw not only visitors from nearby areas, but also from distant areas, due to their unique features or widely known "reputation." The social definition of these places, even more than for district or regional parks, is highly dependent on the activities and attractions to be found there and/or may reflect the particular social definitions and expectations of the dominant user group. Clark and Stankey present a more formal investigative framework called "the recreation opportunity spectrum" [74]. They identify the position of an individual park, or "recreation opportunity," along a 16 continuum by identifying the specific (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) combination of physical, biological, social, and managerial conditions that give value to a place. Thus, an opportunity includes qualities provided by nature (vegetation, landscape, topography, scenery), qualities associated with recreational use (levels and types of use), and conditions provided by management flevelOpments,rOads, regulations) [74, p.l]. six factors they use for classification are: Access -- including both access to the site and within it as well as site location relative to potential users; Nonrecreational land uses -- other resource uses within and around the park which may complement or conflict with recreational use; Onsite management -- the extent, complexity, apparentness, and nature of site modification and facility development; Social interaction -- the number of visitors, their frequency of contact, and the types of contacts and the eXpectations that visitors hold regarding the social milieu of the park; Acceptability of visitor impacts -- essentially a value judgement based on objective measures of impact magnitude (vegetation damage, noise levels, vandalism damage, etc.) and subjective evaluation of their importance; Acceptable regimentation -- management control over visitor behavior, ranging from subtle elements of site design and information programs to official law enforcement activities [74, pp. 8-14]. Table 2.1 combines these three conceptualizations of the park environment "continuum" in simplified form. Thus, a neighborhood "tot lot" would have a low "wildness" content, high levels of accessi- bility, site development, interaction among users, user impacts, and regimentation. Wilderness areas, located at the Opposite end of the 17 mOmuw>fiuom ewes: mo oocoww>o wouwswa mcmnamo O>as coumo mxnwm Hausa .xnmm fie macaw Haasowuhmm povw>oam ouHOm muowumuosooum ucmwnpo>o .uaonmmmm moauw>wuow «:03 lounges mnem>no ecu meowqufimficoa ouwm nu“: macaw HmHSOw: :« woumnucoocoo « :omquwmfiwoa ouwm Acme saw: pomoflo u:oEomn:me cw hawnmawwm mw Ouwm on xma acoemOHo>oa mananopwmcoo AHHmSm: n>Ou Sana“; :oumo Opwmzo mmcwuuom “wean mecca mcwnmuw new cannsnam cw no nonsfiu we seam one umoe oomwocanm mwcwuuom muons gmfiohoasoo mmcwuuom Hansuuzownmm cam cw ovum :OvHoo Hewu:oefimon away: no Hmwucovflmon xuwmcoucw 30H :oumo mm zoom mxhwm zuwo InSm no cash: cw :mnnspsm one away: Om: pcma .OuosOh a“ AHHmst Hemne new pmooxm poumoofi :oumo owes a“ woumoofi xHHaSm: am:0wumouooncoz Ho>mnu xnmm gummy . xomnomnon no «com on muse: Hano>om xnmm gummy oy ecu moawu Ho>mnu fio>wuu xms mnouwmw> on maze: or» can» whom: owes you xnmm wcofi mopflacou :opmo .ousm an Oanwmmooow mmOH Ho>mnu whom: on use: mafia Ho>mau .unommo OHnmnoewmnoo «an oofi>nom awn umoa .ufimcmnu noguo uuozm .ousm no Hoom mowfiscon xamm away“: lamp» mmms anaswou ho pmo an ofinfimmoo an .xhnm canvas can use on :uon mmoou< Haasmon o: coumo tom wamwO Afigesm: Op :uop mmooow xmmm xuflaflnwmmooo< :oflumuomo> monsuaom pomofio>ow Hensum: we ones we Ham: mm mmonm womo~0>oucs ma macaw Humane: :pfiwzz «no: mcfimucoo xmamnma meow :wmucoo an: :eHflz: unwed mmocuaflz manna Omamfi ow scape: omnma ou scape: Hanam ouwm xnmm ceased Hmcowwom pownumwo voosaonnwwoz mowumwnouomnmnu aconHOfi: xaam . Ebahummm >bmz=bm0dmo one=m H.N mgm.z .3om coo nomnozv ucoaaonn>=m one one .moonsomom Hananoziwnon>osom Honoom .nOHAoH poo .xoonu .nonsm on :.mooonm :OMOoonoom noovnsomruzoMuncnmoo Honoom och: .ooa .o.m .E: .nmmémmngmn .moou no.5 £52380 326:: 2F: .533: .2 $8558 mnoms one Ho::Omnom anon coozu top nooucoo oHnunH moono xnom anounoo on pounEnH anon ocu mo .:Onmn>nom=m .:Mnmov 18 can acoao>oe on mnonn unon HoMOnMHpno 3om .nonunoo pooEomocoa poonne mo mHo>oH sou monocomo nonnom om: onmnunss cw mo =Onnosponm no conuoouonm oonsomon now nnwnmannm monocoa on ounm .oonnencna on poomsn nounmn> masonw non: mcoao mpooacoo sow can: xnom canons» on Ame non>o£on now: mo :Onmn>nom:m voonnv .coafionucoo on nos knuao xnoo Hoom unannomEn on :oMnoouonQ ounSOm -on Hanson: :oumo .xonnom nonomo xnom can: kno> mpoomEn mo nunnnno tumoooo woo momxn mxnom nonnumno no voosnopnmnoz on cosy Annonuomm can xfinonomsou goon m:Onunom oEom nmooH no on eomn>nom=m onm mnom: woo oonnonuooo mn xnnco xnom coumo :Onuoononm oonsomon coca nosuon :onn soonoon now vowoooe «mos .xonnom nocomo xnom can: xno> muooman mo Aunanno nunoooo poo momxh mzownno: poo mono uxooz :o Amaonoommo .oom: nan>oo£ annom AHHMSms nan mxnom ounm canons» Honucoo neosomocoa onno unoOnm:oo on noonp3m on nOn>ogop nounmn> wouoomxo on own mo oocoOn>o .om: nounmn> now commons mn ounm mnoms mcoeo mucoucoo :OMHoucoEnmon onpoumooo< muoomEn nounmn> mo nnnfinnonmooo< comnommno ono mnom: connommnc onoa :ngunz coo wooao some coo mnom: zoos :oMnoonoucn .ow: nonmooucn so; on On mono» om: monno> nunmcoucn om: .omo xunmcoucn can: Honoom oposom Ho:Onmoz nonnumno voognonzwnoz monnmnnouoononu Aconononm xnom fl.p:ouv n.~ msm.z .nnmn .mmn-m4n Anon mmnmomom «Hanna; mo naenaon =.on=pnso «unseen cameo: :n connoeoo ego .non>agom .mozna>= .Hnonaaau .m ecu .ooeeo: .u.e .xnmno .z.m .em-mo .os 13oz woo nomnomy usossonn>sm one osolwmoonsomom nonSnoz .nOn>oaom Honoom .noHnon oso .xoonu .nonsm on :.mooomm sonuoonooz nooouso mo conunsnmoa Honoom och: .ooa .u.m ”mmomsom .moono nnuso oso nnopsson xnom an nnso nsooo on oouoosxo .mnom: no wonnmow uoz .mosno> oonosm so oomon .nosnomsa .o:~o> Ongonsnm mo: usossonn>so Honouos on» onogs zansuo nooouso: so ononm on coon mnoms .nsossonn>:o unanss mo omnu nonsonnnom < .nnnoo: o>nH no: no: oo vso oouoosoo unnoz ono mnom: umos .mooosowoso; nanonoom .nsooo nos :monunsos isoo: nnonomsou nan monsumow onaonsnm on emanann on no: .nunnoguso Hosnonxo no oomnonoxo .Hosnom .msonuouoomxo no unon>onon noonoMMno onocm nos masonmnsm .mnom: umos no wsnuuom unansm o wonoonmsoo .mxnom nonnumno nun: m< .msonuooon Honsn on nanosomoso: HonoOm onos .mnom: Hooo~ :sos woo HoooH cuom .monsumom onfionsnm oso noonsoo Hononmnomsm on cannons nnnmsms .nunnocuso nosnouxo onnnmn> nngmn: no connonoxo .Hosnom .mn an moons mo zocnx: on» mo onnmnnouoonoso msno: non>ocon mo roan; may on oumnnnm .wsnuuom unansm o oonoonmsoo nanosms .mnomo nooon ssos oso HoooH Anon .msoosowonouo; nH .HonOOm nno> nHHoSma .ooong manuoos o on nos xnom .oouoomxm .mnoms nosao mo owvonzosx Hosomnom so womon .HosnomsH .QnAmnoszo mo omsom o onosm oso ooomm Hosomnom o xnoa on» noonmsoo nos mnoms .msno: nOn>oson m.ooognon:unos HoOOH on noomnsm soumo .xnom one too: o>nH mnom: nmos msoosowoso: nHHonoom masonm noms msoso sonuoononsH mHonusoo nonoom msnos non>onon xnom nunosowoso: now: ouosoz Hononom connomna woosnopamwoz ngonononm xnom munumnnouoonosu moszPmm Mmzm a<=mm az< q<39mmuzmm mmb N.N mambe safer than those they are less familiar with and urban areas to be less safe than suburban or rural areas [101]. Further, some parks appear to be re- garded as safer than others. This raises the question of relative safety when comparing a particular park to park users' neighborhoods, images of "downtown," and other parks they are aware of. Because research in fear of crime strongly suggests that evenings in any setting are perceived as more dangerous than daytime, it is anticipated that park users will also share this View of temporal differences in park safety. Secondly, research on perception and behavior in many areas, par- ticularly fear of crime, reveals differences among peOple's assessments of personal safety depending on their age, gender, and familiarity with given environments. It is anticipated that women and older people will perceive the park to be less safe and orderly than men or younger people do. It is also expected that (l) the number of depreciative incidents experienced by the respondent as witness, victim, or acquaintance of a victim, (2) frequency of site visits and residential location, and (3) perceived user homogeneity will influence park users' perceptions of park safety. A central question in behavioral geography is the influence of perception on behavior. Both natural hazard research and studies of response to crime find regularities in human re5ponse to various threats. 55 This study addresses most closely the question of how people who perceive a threat in the park environment appear to respond to it, in particular, do they report avoidance behavior? The specific problem statements and hypotheses to be tested are: Problem Statement A: Do park users feel safe in the parks? Hypothesis Al: Hypothesis A2: Hypothesis A3: Park users will report feeling safe in the park. Park users will percieve evenings to be less safe than daytime in the parks. Park users will rate the park as equally or more safe than other parks, their own neighborhood, and downtown areas. Problem Statement B: How do park users' individual characteristics Hypothesis Bl: Subhypothesis relate to their perceptions of park safety and behavioral responses to perceived safety? Park users' perceptions of park safety, in general, are related to their age, gender, and familiarity with the park. Bla: Younger park users will feel safer in the park Subhypothesis than will older users. Blb: Male park users will feel safer in the park Subhypothesis than will female park users. Blc: Park users who are more familiar with the park Hypothesis B2: Hypothesis B3: Hypothesis B4: will feel safer than those who are less familiar with the park. Park users perceiving other park users to be similar to themselves will feel safer than those who perceive other users as different from themselves. Park users who report having been victimized or having witnessed behavior problems in the park or those who report knowing people who have, will report feeling less safe than those who have not had these experiences. Park users who report feeling less safe in the park will also report more avoidance behavior than those users who report higher levels of perceived park safety. Hypothesis B5: Subhypothesis 56 Park users' age, gender, and familiarity with the park are related, in general, to their reported avoidance of some park areas due to safety concerns. BSa: Younger park users will be less likely to Subhypothesis report avoiding some park areas than will older users. BSb: Male park users will be less likely to report Subhypothesis avoiding some park areas than will female users. BSc: Park users who are more familiar with the park Hypothesis B6: Hypothesis B7: will be less likely to report avoiding some park areas or the whole park than those who are less familiar with the park. Park users perceiving other park users to be similar to themselves will be less likely to report avoiding some park areas than those who perceive other users as different from themselves. Park users who report having witnessed behavior problems in the park, or those who report knowing people who have been victimized there, will report more avoidance behavior than will those who have not had these eXperiences. 57 CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY This chapter describes the methods and instruments used to gather the information needed for this study. First, the study parks are described and the reasons for their selection are discussed. Then, the survey instrument and methods used in gathering infOrmation from park users are examined. Study Park Settings Although theories of behavior environment interaction rest on the assumption that people respond to certain behavioral "cues" in different settings, very little is known about the relative importance of various setting components in eliciting behavioral response. Clark and Stankey's recreation opportunity spectrum, coupled with Lee's park hierarchy, suggest what the relevant dimensions of variation may be in park settings, in general. In order to identify more specifically the particular aspects of park environments that influence safety related perceptions and behavior, three similar parks, in terms of their location along the recreation Opportunity spectrum, were selected for study. Research indicates that widely different types of parks tend to have different overall "images" (Table 2.2). Thus, differencesixiperceived safety between, for example, a city park and a wilderness area, might arise more from peoples' implicit theories about what kinds of behavior occur in these types of parks, in general, than to actual differences 58 between a particular pair of park environments. By concentrating the investigation in generally similar "types" of parks this problem is minimized. Further, most research to date has concentrated on either small, neighborhood parks or remote, rural recreation settings such as campgrounds and wilderness areas, while this study concentrates on middle-range district level parks in urban and suburban settings. This facilitates identification of both the relevant environmental charac- teristics that appear to influence park users' evaluations of park safety in all types of park settings and those that differentiate different types of parks' "images." The parks were selected on criteria of (1) location in an urban or suburban area, since these are the areas presumably most affected by fear of crime problems; (2) medium to large size to avoid investigating only "neighborhood" parks and allow for identification of spatial variations within the park; (3) diversity of facilities and, thus, the likelihood of drawing a diversity of user groups; and (4) cooperation of park management personnel. Three parks were chosen: Potter Park in Lansing, Michigan and Elm Creek and Coon Rapids Dam Park Preserves in Hennepin County, Minnesota. Lansing, Michigan, the state capital, is a medium size city in central Michigan. State government and Michigan State University in adjacent East Lansing provide considerable white collar employment, while auto manufacturing supports a large blue collar workforce. Potter Park is located between two older residential areas southeast of the downtown and capital area. The neighborhood on the north side of the park is predominantly lower-middle class and racially mixed. There is a single 59 pedestrian entrance on this side of the park. The only other park en- trance is the single auto access point at the west end of the park, con- necting to a busy four lane street, Pennsylvania Avenue. Some commercial and industrial enterprises are located along this section of Pennsyl- vania Avenue and residential areas across the street from the park are similar, socio-economically, to those at the north boundary. The Red Cedar River forms the southern boundary of Potter Park. Across the Red Cedar is an enclave of medium to high income residences. The natural amenities of the river, nearby parks, and a golfcourse, as well as the quality of the houses in this area has produced a fairly stable, high status neighborhood. Another major street, Aurelius Road, forms the eastern boundary of the park and effectively eliminates either pedestrian or auto access from this direction (Figure 3.1). Potter Park is an older park, and has undergone several design changes through its history. It contains the only free zoo in the mid- Michigan area, although a $1.00 parking fee to drive into the park was instituted, for the first time, in the summer of 1980. The park contains both wooded and open, grassy areas and has extensive picnic facilities -- picnic pavilions, barbeque grills, and numerous picnic tables. There are also playgrounds, tennis courts, a snack stand, a train ride, a river- boat ride, and a canoe rental facility in the park. Park agency person- nel consider Potter to be a family park with regional appeal. They mention that it is a traditional location for many families, organizations, and business groups to hold annual picnics. Because of its attractive natural features and easy access, Potter Park also draws large numbers of teenage and young adult users. In recent years the growing presence of this user group, with their often boisterous and, to some, intimidating 60 fi -- ‘- . . “as ' "nl’fi" ’ 7‘. ~" ‘ n: “fir-vy- -“ T‘L'?‘ ll $11.“fln).fi" ~~J tu- «we: .ey » a u 'fi‘AWO‘h: ’53:.‘h2L 17's; " W ' “n? z ‘0' . ‘ n ‘nfifi ‘3’." . ' {1}?“ ,- II ‘ ::.. 2‘21?" ' ...,-. 2., m \ 'I' fl .q“ 3 :i~~ r .\\\\V t. \ -na O O M .— l.“ “.1 0 § )4 o: «sé go: 3m 9+— u—l— o. AUTOMOBIIE DRIVE AND PARKING -PEDESTRIAN WALK @RAILROAD TRACKS m OPEN wooos [pf-"3:" DENSE BRUSH 7% 200 DISPLAY 61 behavior, has been a source of concern to park management, other users, and adjacent neighbors. Lansing parks are patrolled by a small special division of the city police force. These officers have been forced to spend an increas- ing amount of their time and energy in Potter Park as its popularity as a location for young people to "cruise" and "party" has grown. Complaints of drug and alcohol use, rowdy behavior, and fighting were frequent. Tensions culminated over the Fourth of July holiday in 1980 when a young white man was shot and killed by a young black man, allegedly in response to earlier racial taunts by a group of white youths. Subsequently, the entrance fee was instituted to discourage casual "cruising" and a mounted policeman was employed to patrol the park full-time. Hennepin County, Minnesota, contains both Elm Creek and Coon Rapids Dam Park Preserves. Residential areas adjacent to both parks are primarily suburban, serving a fairly affluent sector of the Minnea- polis area workforce. Elm Creek Park Preserve is a large, newly devel- oped park in a predominately agricultural setting. A sizable, and still growing, middle-class residential subdivision is located along the north- east boundary of the park. Although residents of this development enjoy relatively unobstructed pedestrian and bicycle access to the park, auto access from this side of the park recently was eliminated, creating considerable conflict between local residents and park management (Figure 3.2). As a new park, Elm Creek still has a somewhat "raw" look and . relatively few areas of mature woods. The subsection chosen for analysis in this study contains a swimming pond with lifeguard stations, snack bar, restrooms, beachouse, and a first aid station. Also included is a wooded 62 NM 350.". m>mmmmm v_~_> n:>oo: a 63 picnic area on a hill above the swimming pond (Figure 3.3). Elm Creek, particularly the swimming pond, is a pOpular destination for families and young people throughout the region. Life guards and other personnel provide supervision in the pond area and a small roving force of park rangers provides enforcement coverage for the rest of this and other parks in the Hennepin County Park Preserve system. Coon Rapids Dam Park Preserve is located at the head of navigation on the Mississippi River. It includes wooded riverbank areas on each side of the river and a small power generation dam across the river. The adjacent area on the Hennepin County Side of the river is fairly rural, with low density, lower middle to middle class residential devel- opment of mixed age. The opposite bank, however, borders a small middle class suburban community. The park entrance on this side is within five minutes driving time of the downtown area. There is easy access to the park, both on foot and by car, on both sides of the river (Figure 3.4). This park too is new, at least to the Hennepin County system. Recent acquisition of the site by the Park Preserve District brings both benefits and drawbacks for local users. While the Park Preserve District has invested heavily in site improvements, providing an inter- pretive center, restrooms, picnic and fishing facilities, and improving the aesthetic, accessibility, and safety factors of the dam structure, traditional patterns of access and activity have been disrupted. Parking and entry are now regulated and fees are charged during high use periods. Swimming is prohibited and activities, in general, are more regulated and supervised than was previously the case. Coon Rapids Dam draws local peOple for fishing, picnicking, and general enjoyment of the river and dam. "Sightseeing" by more distant 64 Belch house m~Wooded area Sand beach - Parking area FEET —m FIGURE 3.3 Elm Creek Study Area 6S JJ‘i- f COON RAPIDS DAM REGIONAL PARK FIGURE 3.4 .. .II. ..... ooooo cove ooooo one. ..... s... w Heavily j , WOOdCd - Parking Fishing dock " Hiking / I trail FEET 66 area residents is not uncommon. A small group of South-east Asian immigrants frequently fish here, generating some conflict due to differing language, fishing practices, social norms and customs. The proximity of residential areas and the fairly light level of on-site staffing and park enfOrcement patrols, has led to some problems of "partying" along the riverbanks during evening hours. The three study area parks are fairly close together on the continuum of "recreation opportunities." All might be considered district level parks. Although all three parks are adjacent to residen- tial areas, access to the parks is controlled by park design and management. All three parks are moderately well developed, although the specific attractions vary. Park personnel speak of these parks as "family" parks, but considerable user heterogeneity is visible in each park. All are managed for visitor use, rather than resource protection, and a moderate to high degree of regimentation, or supervision, exists in each park. There is little empirical work available investigating either park user behavior and perceptions or depreciative behavior in these types of settings. Due to time and funding constraints, intensive data collec— tion was limited to three parks in this study. While the data from these three parks have limited generalizability, they do provide baseline information for future research. Comparison of these findings to those of case studies in other types of parks may help identify both common and differentiating elements among park types that influence safety related perceptions and behavior. 67 Park User Survey Measures of park users' perceptions and reported behavior were obtained through on-site interviews. Park user interviews were conducted in a face-to-face verbal fermat with the interviewer recording responses as each question was asked. The survey population was all park users over thirteen years old in the park on interview days. Interview days were selected to provide both weekday and weekend samples and avoid days when special events or conditions might bias responses. A systematic sampling plan was employed to select respondents in the park. Each tenth person over thirteen years old encountered by the interview team while traversing a predetermined route was approached fer an interview. Interviews were conducted in teams, with one team member interviewing the randomly selected respondent while the other team member interviewed the respondent's companion(s). This procedure reduces response contam- ination from the respondents' companions. Only randomly selected respondents' answers are included in the statistical analyses. Inter- views lasted from five to twenty minutes. Since less than five percent of all users approached in the survey refused to be interviewed, no measures of refusal bias are calculated. When the selected respondent did refuse, the next individual encountered along the route was inter- viewed. The survey instrument and administration techniques were pre- tested in Potter Park during the spring of 1980. The sampling technique is an adaptation of one deveIOped by the Hennepin County Park Preserve District research staff. Selecting the statistically correct respondent from among a group of people was sometimes difficult. When in doubt, interviewers attempted to choose the respondent that provided the most diversity to the sample, 68 i.e. the one representing a subgroup less frequently encountered. This is appropriate given the exploratory nature of the study and the need for information about the range of responses possible. Also, people who were clearly in transit from one area to another (into or out of parked cars or restrooms, for example) were excluded from the sample selection count. It was felt, based on pre-test experiences, that this avoided many potential rersals without seriously affecting the representative- ness of the sample. Sooner or later people became reasonably stationary and approachable, re-entering the sample population. Overall, given the time, funding, and situational constraints of determining and "capturing" random respondents in an unregulated, free entry and open movement environment, this plan seems to have worked fairly well in terms of efficiency and effectiveness.in providing a cross-section of park users. Table 3.1 illustrates the demographic characteristics of the park users surveyed. To insure that the study results are reflections of reality and not products of interviewer bias several measures of interviewer bias were calculated (Table 3.2). The interviewers in each park were cross-tabulated against the responses they recorded on each of several questions. The items used for evaluation of potential bias are some of those that are most important in the relationships reported regarding park user perceptions and behavior. There are no significant differences as measured by chi-square measures of association among interviewers on any of these items. Further, knowing the interviewer's identity does not reduce the predictive uncertainty in any of the items by more than eight percent as measured by the Asymmetric Uncertainty Coeffecient 69 TABLE 3.1 DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF PARK USER SURVEY SAMPLE Age: Gender: Race: Characteristic 13-21 years 22—34 years 35-54 years Over 54 years No response Male Female Missing White Nonwhite* Education: Less than high school High school graduate 1-4 years of college More than 4 years of college Household Annual Income: Less than $7,500 $7,500 - $15,000 $15,000 - $25,000 $25,000 - $35,000 Over $35,000 No response Work Status: Unemployed or retired Student Homemaker White collar worker Blue collar worker No response Number 77 109 43 12 17 120 137 228 30 49 78 79 SI 31 43 40 134 34 39 36 95 50 29. 42. 16. 4. 6. 46. 53. 0. 88. 11. 19. 30. 30. 19. 12. 16. 15. 51. 13. 15. 14. 36. 19. U‘thOi—‘N Percent mO‘NO #i—‘U‘ O‘NVNO KOU'INOHW * Includes blacks, hispanics, and orientals 70 TABLE 3 .2 MEASURES OF INTERVIEWER BIAS: PARK USER SURVEY Park Potter Elm Creek Coon Rapids Dam User Response Measures , , . nu nu nu OH: O OH: O OH: d) -H s o n -H s o n on s o n I»: or). «4 o H w-i «4 o H -:-I 0H a: uoo :3 uoo :3 uoo :3 GH-H c"! QH-r—l Cris own-I Cr-I: EH44 me gnu-f m-I: gnu-I :04: 0‘44 a 0'4». rs 0'44 fl mug-He .Ugwflcnog "43 .58 e 250. 5 <58 5 Fear index scores 06 23 .00 .99 .004 .65 Daytime avoidance O4 23 .08 .07 .0002 .97 Evening avoidance .02 .42 .009 .46 .01 .39 Number of different types of depreciative behavior ' witnessed in park .05 .21 .01 .84 4.05 .15 Number of crimes and/or harassment incidents reported .07 .42 .001 .93 .05 .65 Frequency of seeing user behavior problems .03 .47 .05 .14 .04 .34 Frequency of seeing litter and vandalism .04 .31 .03 .53 .0002 .89 Frequency of seeing rule violations .05 .28 .04 .30 .02 .39 Age of respondent .04 .17 .04 .16 .Ol .55 Visit frequency of respondent .06 .31 .03 .39 .Ol .56 Travel time home to park for ' _ respondent .04 .74 .04 .21 .01 .74 * Asymmetric Uncertainty Coefficient with interviewer identity as the independent variable ** Statistical significance level of Chi-square measure of association 71 (Table 3.2). Thus, it is reasonably certain that any relationships among surveyed park users' individual characteristics, park location, perceptions of safety, and avoidance behavior cannot be attributed to interviewer bias. 72 CHAPTER FOUR: DATA ANALYSIS In this chapter the research questions are addressed and the hypotheses tested. The first two sections discuss how park users view the parks in terms of relative safety and how their individual character— istics influence their assessments of park safety and behavior in the park. The final section provides a list of each hypothesis and sub- hypothesis indicating whether they are accepted or rejected. Relative Park Safety Assessments of park safety were obtained by asking survey res- pondents whether they would feel safe alone in the park during the day and evening. Over ninety percent of those questioned reported feeling safe in the park during the day. Evening hours, however, are considered much less safe with over forty percent of the respondents reporting that they would not feel safe alone in the park during the evening (Table 4.1). The question of safety in the park relative to safety in other public settings was addressed through asking respondents to compare their feelings of being safe in the park to how safe they feel in their own neighborhoods and downtown areas. Respondents were also asked to compare the study park to other parks they knew about in terms of problems like people breaking the law or park rules. As Table 4.2 illustrates, the study parks were overwhelmingly rated as safer than 73 TABLE 4.1 PARK SAFETY DURING DAY AND EVENING Response Question Yes No N (%) N ('6) Would you feel safe spending the day alone in this park? 232 (91.3) 22 ( 8.7) Would you feel safe spending the evening alone in this park?* ‘ 136 (57.9) 96 (40.9) * "Depends on" response category deleted (1.2 percent of responses) 74 TABLE 4.2 RELATIVE PARK SAFETY Response Question Better Same Worse N(%) N05) N06) Park compared to downtown 96 (73.3) 25 (19.1) 20 ( 7.6) Park compared to other parks 92 (40.2) ' 122 (53.3) 15 ( 6.6) Park compared to neighborhood 26 (19.7) 48 (36.4) 58 (43.9) 75 downtown areas (73.3 percent "better" and 19.1 percent ”same" ratings). Also, over ninety percent of those questioned felt that crime problems and rule infractions in the study park were no more (53.3 percent "same") or less (40.2 percent "better")severethan those in other parks (Table 4.2). Perceptions of park safety relative to safety in the respondents' neighborhoods are less positive. Although the majority (56.1 percent) of survey respondents report feeling at least as safe in the park as in their own neighborhood, a substantial minority (43.9 percent) feel the park is less safe than their neighborhood (Table 4.2). These results are consistent with other research which suggests that perceptions of safety are related to an individual's familiarity with the setting in question [101]. Based on these findings Hypotheses Al through A3 are accepted. Park users do appear to regard the park as a fairly safe environment, particularly during the day (Hypothesis Al). Evenings are considered less safe than daytime in the parks (Hypothesis A2) and most users do rate the park as at least as safe as downtown areas, other parks, and their own neighborhoods (Hypothesis A3). The decrease in perceived levels of park safety during the evening and relative to respondents' home neighborhoods is more logically attributable to overall assess- ments of safety based on the time of day and familiarity with the environment in question than to the specific park environment [101,119]. The importance of temporal setting is revealed in Law Enforcement Assistance Administration survey responses indicating that 26 to 47 percent of residents interviewed in five U.S. cities do not feel safe alone in their own neighborhoods at night [111,112,113,ll4,115]. 76 In order to assess the relationships between both personal and setting factors and park users' perceptions of park safety a composite measure of perceived safety was constructed. This four point, ordinal level, "fear index" is composed of responses to three questionnaire items: the two previously discussed questions about feeling safe alone in the park during the day and evening and a question about whether or not the respondent feels other people in the park are well behaved. Guttman scalogram analysis of the responses to these questions yields a coefficient of reproducibility of .99 and a coefficient of scalability of .95, both well above the minimum values required for a valid scale [146]. There are only six scalogram errors, indicating only three of the respondents answering these questions did not conform to the pattern. This suggests that all three items tap a single dimension of variation within park users' appraisals of the park. However, since the Guttman scale contains fewer than the ideal minimum of twelve items, the "fear index" scores computed from these items are treated as ordinal measures. The four item fear index varies from 1, for respondents who feel other park users are well behaved and that the park is safe for a person alone both day and night, to 2 for people who feel other users are well behaved but who do not feel safeiJIthe park alone during the evening, to 3 for those finding park users well behaved but not feeling safe alone in the park either day or night, and finally, to 4 for those individuals finding other park users to be not very well behaved and the park to be unsafe at all times for a person alone. Table 4.3 indicates the number of respondents in each category. 77 TABLE 4.3 FEAR INDEX SCORES Score Responses N (%) 1 Other users are well behaved, I feel safe alone in the park day and night 134 (58.0) 2 Other users are well behaved, I feel safe alone in the park during the day but not at night 75 (32.5) 3 Other users are well behaved, I do not feel safe alone in the park day or night 17 ( 7.4) 4 Other users are not well behaved, I do not feel safe alone in the park day or night 5 ( 2.2) Total 231 (100.0) 78 Due to the low number of respondents with the maximum index score of four (only five people) scale steps three and four are com- bined for subsequent analyses, yielding an index value ranging from one to three. Association of Park UserS' Individual Characteristics with Safety_Related Perceptions and Behavior Age, Gender, Familiarity, and Perceptions of Safety Research on fear of crime suggests that age, gender, and familiar- ity with an environment are important factors in assessments of personal safety. In order to evaluate the association of each of these character- istics with park users' perceptions of safety, as indicated by their "fear index" score, two measures of association were calculated: the Asymmetric Uncertainty Coefficient and Kendall's Tau. Both measures vary from 0 (no association) to 1 (perfect correspondence). The Asym- metric Uncertainty Coefficient is designed for nominal level variables and measures the proportion by which "uncertainty" in the dependent variable is reduced by knowledge of the independent variable [146, p.226]. It is based on the concept of "proportional reduction in error" and considers the entire distribution of the data, rather than just the modal category. Kendall's Tau does not assume a directional relationship and measures the strength of association between two ordinal level variables by considering their order in every possible pair of cases [146, p.227]. The Tau statistic may be negative or positive and the probability of the observed association arising by chance can be calcul- ated. Use of these two measures affords an estimate of the variation 79 explained in the dependent variable by knowing the value of the independent variable as well as an indication of the strength and statistical significance of their association.- Hypotheses Bl through B7 are accepted when (1) the calculated Kendall's Tau is both statistically significant at the .05 level or higher and greater than or equal to .30 and (2) the Asymmetric Uncertainty Coefficient is .10 or greater, indicating at least a ten percent improvement in predicting the dependent variable when the value of the independent variable is known. Establishing these "cut-off" levels reduces the probability of accepting a false hypothesis based on significant but weak levels of association and focuses the analysis on the stronger relationships apparent in the data. In order to test for possible relationships between respondents' age and perceptions of park safety, each respondent's age was classified into one of four ordinal age categories. As the figures in Table 4.4 demonstrate, age is significantly related to perceptions of park safety; as expected, fear index scores tend to increase with respondents' age, however the relationship is not strong enough to accept subhypothesis Bla. Only five percent of the variation in fear scores is explained by age alone. While the proportion of respondents who report feeling unsafe alone in the parks does increase with age, especially for respondents over 55 years old, the low number of respondents in this oldest age group reduces both the reliability and generalizability of these results (Table 4.5). Gender, a dichotomous, metric level variable, however, is both significantly and strongly related to perceptions of park safety (Kendall's Tau of .64) with females having consistently higher "fear ASSOCIATION OF RESPONDENTS' 80 TABLE 4.4 WITH FEAR INDEX SCORES PERSONAL FACTORS Asymmetric Uncer- I Kendall's Tau Personal Factors N tainty Coefficient* (p) Age 217 f .05 .11 (.02)** Gender 230 .26 .64 (.00001)** Familiarity with park: Distance from home to park 230 .01 .10 (.05)** Frequency of park visits 231 .04 .ll (.02)** Length of local residence 226 .02 .00 (.47) * Fear index dependent ** Statistically significant at .05 or higher level 81 TABLE 4.5 AGE AND GENDER OF RESPONDENTS WHO REPORT FEELING UNSAFE IN THE PARKS Respondents with: Fear index score Fear index score "' ”.=3 2' , =2, ' Age and Total ‘TT Gender N (9) % all % all % all all Classes ° with age with age N score or N score or 3 gender 2 ender _ class' . class Age (years) 13—21 69 (31.8) 4 20.0 5.8 22 30.6 32.4 22-34 4 99 (45.6) 8 40.0 8.1 31 43.1 32.3 35-54 41 (18.9) 4 20.0 9.8 18 25.0 43.9 Over 55 8 ( 3.7) 4 20.0 50.0 1 1.4 12.5 . Total 217(100.0) 20 100.0 9.2 72 100.0 33.2 Gender Male 107 (46.5) 0 0.0 0.0 9 9.3 8.4 Female. _ . 123 (53.5) 22 100.0 17.9 88 90.7 71.5 .fTotal” 230(100.0) 22 100.0 9.6 97 100.0 32.5 82 index" scores than do male respondents. Over one fourth of the varia- tion in fear scores is predicted by gender (Table 4.4). None of the male survev respondents report feeling unsafe alone in the park during both day and evening and only 8.4 percent report feeling unsafe during the evening (Table 4.5). Thus, subhypothesis Blb is accepted. Three ordinal level operational measures of park users' familiar- ity with the study parks were evaluated against fear index scores: (1) the distance a park user lives from the park, as measured by travel time from home to park; (2) how often the park user visits the study park; and (3) how long the user has lived in the area. Familiarity, as measured by distance from park to home and frequency of park visits, is signifi- cantly related to fear index scores, although weakly. Less than five percent of the variation in fear index scores is explained by each of these measures (Table 4.4). Lenth of local residence is not significantly associated with park users' assessments of personal safety in the park. Those who visit the park more frequently and/or live closer to the park are Somewhat less likely to report feeling unsafe in the park than those who visit it less often and/or live further from the park, although, again, the association is too weak for hypothesis acceptance. Subhypo- thesis Blc is therefore rejected. Table 4.6 illustrates the proportion of residents in each travel time and visit frequency category reporting feeling unsafe alone in the park. Perceived Social Homogeneity and Perceptions of Safety Several researchers have postulated that feelings of safety are related to the perceived social homogeneity of a setting [73,101]. It is suggested that when peOple perceive others to be similar to themselves, 83 TABLE 4.6 RESIDENTIAL PROXIMITY AND PARK VISIT FREQUENCY OF RESPONDENTS WHO REPORT FEELING UNSAFE ALONE IN THE PARKS Respondents with: . Fear index score ‘1 Fear index score Proximity and Total =3 . 2 :2 Frequency . 1 I ~ . . % all %.of -. % all % of % Classes ' N ( 3 N with all N with all j score in : score : in 3‘ class - 2 Elass Residential Proximity (Travel time): 1-15 minutes . 136 (59.1) 12 54.5 8.8 40 53.3 29.4 16-30 minutes 1 63 (27.4) *5 22.7 7.9 23' 30.7 36.5 Over 31 minutes 31 (13.5) '5 22.7 16.1 12 16.0 38.7 Total 5 230(100.0) 22 100.0 9.6 75 100.0 32.6 Summer Visit Frequency:; 5 or more visits/month ' 70 (30.3) ’5 22.7 7.1 14 18.7. 20.0 1—4 visits/month 51 (22.1) 5 22.7 9.8 21 28.0 41.2 Fewer than 1 visit/month 110 (47.6) 12‘ 54.5 10.9 40 53.3 36.4 Total 231(100.0) 22 100.0 9.5 75 100.0 32.5 84 the predictability of the behavior of other setting participants is increased and the individual feels more comfortable and, presumably, more safe and secure. The dichotomous, metric level, operational measures used to indicate perceived social homogeneity in this study are: (1) race of the survey respondent; (2) responses to a survey question asking whether the respondent feels that most other park users share their values; and (3) responses to a survey question regarding whether or not park users "watch out for each other" or "don't want to get involved." This question is intended to indicate park users' assessments of the level of informal social controls in the park. As Table 4.7 illustrates, none of these measures are significantly related to perceptions of park safety and none explain more than two per- cent of the variation in fear scores. Thus, the hypothesis that users perceiving other park users to be similar to themselves is rejected. Only 13.6 percent of the survey respondents felt that other people in the park did not share their values. Almost one fourth of the respon- dents (24.8 percent) report that people in the park "don't want to get involved" with others. Experiences with Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Perceptions of Safety It may be that people who have seen, been the victim of, or know a victim of a crime or other antisocial behavior in the parks will feel less safe in the park than those not having these experiences. In order to evaluate the associations between these factors and perceptions of safety, survey respondents were questioned about how frequently, on an ordinal level scale, they had seen or experienced each of several 85 TABLE 4.7 THE ASSOCIATION OF PERCEPTIONS OF SOCIAL HOMOGENEITY WITH PERCEPTIONS OF PARK SAFETY Asymmetric Uncer- Kendall's Social Homogeneity N tainty Coefficient Tau Measure with Fear Index (p) Score Dependent Race 230 .01 -.09 (.09) Other users have similar values to respondent's 215 .02 .00 (.50) Park users watch out for each other 183 .001 .04 (.32) 86 types of antisocial or depreciative behavior. Table 4.8 illustrates the relationships between respondents' fear index scores and their exper- ience with various types of park behavior problems. The only types of depreciative behavior that are significantly related to perceptions of park safety are alcohol and drug use. However, these relationships are weak and in the opposite direction from that expected. Thus, the hypo- thesis that park users who report having been victimized or having witnessed behavior problems in the park or those who report knowing peOple who have, will report feeling less safe in the park than those who have not had these experiences is not supported and must be rejected. Fewer than half of the respondents who report eXperience with serious crime in the park also report feeling unsafe alone there (Table 4.9). Only about one third of those who report Voften" seeing other users drinking alcohol or using drugs in the park report feeling unsafe (Table 4.9). In fact, the proportion of respondents who perceive the park to be unsafe tends to decrease as their reported frequency of seeing alcohol and drug use in the parks increases. This apparently contradictory, or at least unexpected, association between witnessing alcohol and drug consumption and perceptions of park safety requires fUrther exploration. Inspection of Table 4.10 reveals that park user age is significantly related to both frequency of park visitation and frequency of experiencing all types of antisocial behavior. Over 90 percent of the users who report visiting the park more than five times a month are under 34 years old, 43.5 percent are under 21. Ninety percent of the serious crime and harassment incidents are reported by park users under 34 years old; nearly half are reported by users between 13 and 21. Similarly, all the users who report frequently witnessing user 87 ' TABLE 4.8 ASSOCIATION OF WITNESSING ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN THE PARK WITH PERCEPTIONS OF SAFETY (N = 244) Asymmetric Uncer- Kendall's Type of Antisocial Behavior ta1nty Coeff1c1ent Tau (Fear score. (P) dependent) Number of serious crimes or harassment incidents reported* .01 -.004 (.48) Frequency of Witnessing: Littering .01 .06 (.11) Park vandalism .03 -.005 (.41) Rowdy behavior .02 .02 (.31) Traffic violations .02 .05 (.17) Damage to user property .07 .03 (.32) Alcohol consumption .05 -.20 (.00001)** Drug use . .05 -.07 (.05)** Rule violations .01 -,oz (.31) * N=231 ** Significant at or above the .05 level TABLE 4.9 88 THE EXPERIENCE OF RESPONDENTS WHO REPORT FEELING UNSAFE ALONE IN THE PARK WITH CRIME, ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION, AND DRUGS IN THE PARKS Respondents with: Fear index score Fear index score Frequency of Total = 3 = 2 Experience ~ N (%) % all % of % all % of N. with 11 N with' all . score in ' score in 3 class 2 :class Serious crime or harassment: O incidents reported 203 (87.9) 20 90. 9.9 65 86.7 32. l or more incidents . : i reported 28 (12.1) 2 9. 7.1 ‘10 13.3 35. Total 231(100.0) 22 100. 9.5 :75[100.0 32. Have seen pegple drinkingfialcohol: Never 122 (so 0) 15 65.2 12.3 51 63.0 41. Once or a few times 57 (23 4) 3 13.0 5.3 '20 24.7 35.1 Often 65 (26 6) 21.7 7.7 :10 12.3 15. Total 244(100.O) 23 100. 9.4 81 100.0 33. Have seen pegple using drugs: Never 185 (75.8) 17 73. 9.2 67 82.7 36.2 Once or a few times 29 (11.9) 4 17.4 13.8 9 11.1 31.0 Often 30 (12.3) 8. 6.7 6.2 16.7 Total 244(100.0) 23 100. 9.4 81 100.0 33. 89 TABLE 4.10 ASSOCIATION OF AGE WITH WITNESSING ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN THE PARKS AND FREQUENCY OF PARK VISITATION (N = 241) Asymmetric Uncer- Kendall's “W. °.f .. “2“.‘2226222765‘ _ 233’ independent Number of serious crimes or harassment incidents reported .04 , -.08 (.Ol)* NUmber of types of antisocial behavior witnessed .06 -.28 (.0001)* Frgguency of witnessing: Rule violations (including traffic violations) .07 -.25 (.0001)* User behavior problems (including alcohol and drug use, rowdy behavior, damage to user property) .06 , -.24 (.0001)* Litter and vandalism .04 2 4.17 (.0001)* Frequency of visiting the park .04 .22 (.0001)* *Statistically significant at .05 level or higher 9O behaviorpucblems,including alcohol and drug consumption as well as rowdy behavior and damage to user property, are under 34 and 60 percent are under 21 (Table 4.11). These incidents do not appear to influence younger park users' assessments of their personal safety in the park. Possibly younger users do not perceive these behaviors as personally threatening or as "problems," particularly those incidents defined as "user behavior problems." Safety Perceptions and Avoidance Behavior Hypothesis B4, which states that park users who report feeling less safe in the park will also report more avoidance behavior than those users who report higher levels of perceived park safety, directly addresses the question of the association between perception and behavior. Park users were asked whether or not they would (or did) avoid particular areas within the park, during the day or evening, due to safety concerns. Respondents answering "yes" were classified as exhibiting "avoidance behavior." Two dichotomous, metric level, measures of avoidance were obtained, treating day and evening behavior separately. Comparing these responses to individuals' fear index scores in Table 4.12 reveals a moderate to strong and statistically significant relationship between perceptions of safety and reported avoidance behavior, during both day and evening. The relationship between perceived safety and reported avoidance behavior during the evening is the stronger of the two (Kendall's Tau = .59). Knowledge of a respondent's "fear score" reduces the un- certainty of their reported avoidance by 16 to 32 percent. Based on these relationships, Hypothesis B4 is accepted. It is interesting to note, however, that 20 to 25 percent of those who report avoidance behavior also 91 TABLE 4.11 FREQUENCY OF EXPERIENCING SERIOUS CRIME, USER BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS, AND PARK VISITATION FOR EACH AGE CATEGORY Percent of each frequency class in Frequency of Total each age group (age in years) Experience N (%) 13-21 22-34 35-54 Over 55 (N=77) (N=109) (N=113) (N=12) Serious crime or harassment: O incidents reported 211 (87.5) 29.9 45.5 19.0 5.7 l or more incidents reported 30 (12.5) 46.7 43.3 10.0 0.0 User behavior problems seen:* Never 104 (43.2) 21.2 46.2 26.0 6.7 Once or seldom 84 (34.9) 32.1 45.2 16.7 6.0 A few times 23 ( 9.5) 43.5 47.8 .7 0.0 Often 30 (12.4) 60.0 40.0 .0 0.0 Visit frequency:** 5 or more visits/month 69 (28.6) 43.5 47.8 7.2 1-4 visits/month 56 (23.2) 32.1 48.2 14.3 Fewer than 1 visit/month 116 (48.1) 25.0 42.2 25.9 Total 241(100.0) 31.9 45.2 17.8 5.0 * Includes alcohol consumption, drug use, rowdy behavior, and damage to park user property ** Frequency of visiting park during the summer 92 TABLE 4.12 ASSOCIATION OF PERCEPTIONS OF SAFETY AND AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR (N = 221) Asymmetric Uncer- Kendall's . . . tainty Coefficient Tau Av01ds parts of the park dur1ng. with avoidance (p) dependent Daytime .16 .33 (.0001)* Evening .32 .59 (.0001)* * Statistically significant at .0001 or above 93 report feeling safe alone in the park during both day and evening hours (Table 4.13). When asked what areas of the park they avoided, 3.5 percent of the respondents (9 people) indicated they do or would avoid the park entirely during the day due to safety concerns while 12 percent (31 people) report avoiding the whole park during the evening for this reason. Thirty-two percent of those who report evening avoidance and forty-two percent of those reporting daytime avoidance behavior say they do so because the park is not a safe place for a person to be alone, particularly during certain hours. The fact that so many of the reasons given for avoiding the park involve being alone suggests that respondents may have been confused by the question order. Apparently, the previous question regarding whether or not the respondent feels safe alone in the park biased at least some answers to the next set of questions regarding whether or not they ever avoided certain portions of the park. The avoidance questions did not ask about behavior only when the respondent was alone. Survey respondents were also asked, in a different portion of the interview, whether they had ever visited the park during the evening. Crosstabluation of these responses against measures of safety perception and reported behavior does not reveal any strong relationships (Table 4.14). This provides further evidence that respondents who reported avoiding the whole park during the evening may have been thinking of not visiting the park alone, rather than not visiting it at all. On the other hand, it is possible that respondents who report avoiding the park in the evening do so based on their experiences from previous evening visits. 94 TABLE 4.13 PROPORTION OF RESPONDENTS WHO REPORT AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR AND THEIR FEAR INDEX SCORES Report avoidance during: Fear Index Score N (6) Daytime Evening N (%)* ' N (%)* l 130 ( 58.8) 9 (20.5) 25 (24.7) 2 70 ( 31.7) 24 (54.5) 62 (61.5) 3 21 ( 9.5) 11 (25.0) 14 (13.9) Total 221 (100.0) 44 (19.9)** 101 (45.7)** * Percent of all who report avoidance behavior ** Percent of all respondents 95 TABLE 4.14 ASSOCIATION OF SAFETY RELATED PERCEPTIONS AND BEHAVIOR AND REPORTED EVENING VISITATION I ) I ' Respondent has visited the park during the evening Perceived Safety and Avoidance N Asymmetric Kendall's Behavior Uncertainty Tau Coefficient* (p) Fear index score 243 .012 -.10 (.06) Avoidance behavior during daytime 251 .0002 .02 (.40) Avoidance behavior during evening 242 .00002 .01 (.47) * Evening visits dependent on safety and avoidance measures 96 Personal Characteristics and Avoidance Behavior Turning to the relationships between personal characteristics, perceptions, and experiences and avoidance behavior, gender is again the strongest predictor, particularly of reported avoidance of park areas during evening hours (Table 4.15). Knowledge of a respondent's gender reduces the uncertainty of predicting whether they will report avoiding the park or areas in the park by 11 to 30 percent (Table 4.15). Females comprise about 85 percent of those reporting both day and evening avoid- ance and over three-fourths of the female respondents report avoiding the park or areas in the park during the evening (Table 4.16). Age, familiarity with the park, race, and perceptions of social homogeneity are not significantly related to either day or evening avoidance behavior (Table 4.15). From this evidence subhypothesis 85b that female park users will report more avoidance behavior than will male park users is accepted while the subhypotheses relating avoidance behavior to age, familiarity with the park, and perceived user homogeneity are rejected. Experiences with Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Reported Avoidance The relationship between observed or experienced antisocial behavior and reported avoidance is illustrated in Table 4.17. Experience with serious crimes such as robbery, assault, theft, intimidation, or fights among park users is significantly associated with reported avoid- ance behavior, both day and evening, however the relationship is weak and less than five percent of the variance in avoidance is explained by exposure to crime. Between 38 and 67 percent of the respondents who ASSOCIATION OF RESPONDENTS' 97 TABLE 4.15 PERSONAL CHARACTERISTICS AND PERCEPTIONS OF PARK SOCIAL HOMOGENEITY WITH AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR , Respondent reports avoidance during: Daytime Evening Personal Characteristics o 5“; o 5‘ E «4:0 in «'17-‘49 In H'H-H - Hort-H — H 83.3 2 H 233.3 :1 .8 E84“: .8 .8 are 8 5 °8 8:4 I; W: 88 52 ‘2:§c9 =KE~ 12:5:3 :45— Age 226 .05 .05 J 218 .02 -.07 Gender 241 .ll .31**i 232 .30 .62** Familiarityi Distance from park to home 241 .01 .07 232 .002 .04 Frequency of park visits 242 .003 .01 A 233 .002 .03 Length of local residence 237 .02 .07 228 .005 .02 Social homogeneity: Race 241 .006 .07 N 232 .004 -.07 Other respondents have similar values 221 .001 .02 216 .01 -.10 Park users "watch out" for each other 185 .002 .04 182 .0001 .02 * Avoidance behavior dependent on personal factors ** Kendall's Tau significant at .05 or higher level 98 TABLE 4.16 PROPORTION OF MALE AND FEMALE RESPONDENTS REPORTING AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR M§l£§_ Females ERIE}. Daytime Avoidance: Total number 113 128 241 Percent of all respondents 46.9 53.1 100.0 Number reporting daytime avoidance 7 39 46 Percent all reporting daytime avoidance 15.2 84.8 100.0 Percent all in gender 6.2 30.5 19.1 Evening_Avoidance Total number 108 124 232 Percent of all respondents 46.6 53.4 100.0 Number reporting evening avoidance 15 94 109 Percent all reporting evening avoidance 13.8 86.2 100.0 Percent all in gender 13.9 75.8 47.0 99 TABLE 4.17 ASSOCIATION OF WITNESSING ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR WITH REPORTED AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR .Respondent reports avoidance during: Daytime Evening (N=252) (N=243) . '3 4: Type of Antisoc1al >3) >3: - H Behav1or .3 +5 5 1” 2 2 5 i” “fin-4 H Hod-H H #60 H uoo H ”to 881?. ”7.12.": 888. Emu sew Eom :95: °8 2 °8 .2 25o 25o Number of serious crime or harassment incidents reported .05 .01 (.002)* .02 .09 (.01)* Frequency of witnessing: Littering .05 .11 (.009)* .02 .16 (.007)* Park vandalism .02 .14 (.05)* .01 .01 (.32) Rowdy behavior .02 .09 (.04)* .01 .08 (.09) Traffic violations .03 .13 (.0004)* .03 .19 (.002)* Damage to user prOperty .01 .01 (.19) .01 .03 (.07) Alcohol consumption .01 -,03 (.29) .01 -.ll (.06) Drug use .01 .02 (.33) .03 -.02 (.36) Rule violations .01 .06 (.12) .00 .003(.48) * Significant at .05 or higher level ** Avoidance behavior dependent 100 report being victimized in the park or knowing someone who was also report avoidance behavior (Table 4.18). Weak but statistically significant associations also exist between reported avoidance behavior and the frequency of witnessing litter and vandalism problems, rowdy behavior, and traffic violations. Although none of these factors reduces the predictive "uncertainty" of avoidance behavior by more than five percent, they do tend to reinforce conventional wisdom regarding the negative influence of physical park damage and rowdy behavior on park user perceptions of social order in the park setting [8,128,135,147,148]. Still, in each category, at least half of the respondents reporting avoidance behavior report never witnessing these depreciative behaviors (Table 4.18). Further, only 14 (5.6 percent) of those interviewed report witnessing park vandalism and while half of them also report evening avoidance this still constitutes only 7 people -- six percent of all those who report avoiding the park or areas in the park during the evening. The numbers of people report- ing having witnessed litter and/or rowdy behavior are larger -- about 35 percent of those interviewed -- and between 53 and 47 percent of them also report evening avoidance. These proportions drop to 24 and 27 percent for reported daytime avoidance (Table 4.18). The association between avoidance behavior and the reported frequency of witnessing traffic violations, although weak, suggests that respondents were not thinking only of the threat of being victimized or harassed when reporting areas they would avoid due to safety concerns (Table 4.18). The threat of being hit by a careless driver may be a component in avoiding certain park areas. Over sixty percent of those respondents who report witnessing traffic violations "Often" also report 101 TABLE 4.18 THE EXPERIENCE OF RESPONDENTS REPORTING AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR WITH CRIME, LITTER, VANDALISM,.ROWDY BEHAVIOR AND TRAFFIC VIOLATIONS IN THE PARK Respondent reports avoidance during: Daytime Evening Frequency Total (N=48)* (N=117)* Classes ResP°nd' % all % all % all % all gngg) N who in N who in av01d class av01d class Serious crime or harassment: 0 incidents reported 213 (88.0) 35 76.1 16.4 91 83.5 44.2 1 or more incidents reported 21 (12.0) 11 23.9 37.9 18 16.5 66.7 Littering: Never seen 164 (65.1) 24 50.0 14.6 67 57.3 42.4 Once/few times 64 (25.4) 19 39.6 33.3 36 30.8 56.2 Often seen 24 ( 9.5) 5 10.4 20.8 14 12.0 58.3 Park vandalism: Never seen 238 (94.4) 43 89.6 18.1 110 94.0 47.8 Once/few times 9 ( 3.6) 22.2 5 4. 55.6 Often seen 5 ( 2.0) 60.3 2 66.7 Rowdy behavior: Never seen 166 (65.9) 27 56.3 16.3 71 60.7 44.7 Once/few times 63 (25.0) 14 29.2 22.2 35 29.9 55.6 Often seen 23 ( 9.1) 7 14.6 30.4 11 9. 55.0 Traffic violations: Never seen 157 (62.3) 23 47.9 14.6 62 53.0 40.5 Once/few times 64 (25.4) 14 29.2 21.9 38 32.5 59.4 Often seen 31 (12.3) 11 22.9 35.5 17 14.5 60.7 *N=46 daytime and N=109 evening for serious crime category 102 avoiding areas of the park during the evening and 26.3 percent report daytime avoidance (Table 4.18). The frequency of witnessing rule viol- ations or user behavior problems is not associated with park users' decisions to avoid certain areas of the park or the whole park during either day or evening hours. Although it does appear that park users who report having witnessed behavior problems in the park, or those who report knowing people who have been victimized there, are somewhat more likely to report avoidance behavior than will those who have not had these experiences, Hypothesis B7 does not receive sufficient support for acceptance and must be rejected. One of the problems in assessing the relationship between experience with antisocial behavior in the park and both perceptions of safety and reported avoidance is the low number of respondents who report experiencing, or knowing someone who has experienced, a serious crime or incident in the parks. Only four survey respondents (1.6 percent of the random sample) report knowing of a violent crime in the park; none were experienced personally by the respondent (Table 4.19). Eight people (3.2 percent) report park thefts and twenty-four people (9.5 percent) report incidents of intimidation, "hassling," or fights among park users. Possibly a larger sample would reveal stronger relationships between perception and criminal victimization. Nearly three-fourths (74 percent) of the respondents report witnessing at least one type of problem behavior in the park. Alcohol consumption is the most frequently reported incident observed with 48.4 percent of the respondents reporting seeing people drinking alcohol in the park, followed by rule violations (35.7 percent) and parking or traffic violations (37.7 percent). One third of the respondents report 103 TABLE 4.19 REPORTS OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR WITNESSED IN THE PARKS (N = 252) Type of Antisocial Behavior Numbgr Percent Respondent experienced or knows someone who did: Violent crime 4 1.6 Theft 8 3.2 Intimidation, fights, argument, or "hassle" 24 9.5 Respondent has seen: Littering 88 34.9 Park vandalism 16 6.3 Violation of pet/leash rules 73 29.0 Rowdy, loud, or offensive behavior 86 34.1 Parking or traffic violations 95 37.7 Damage to park user property 6 2.4 Drinking alcohol 122 48.4 Drug use 60 23.8 Rule violations 90 35.7 104 witnessing littering and rowdy or offensive behavior in the park. Damage of park user property (2.4 percent) and park vandalism (6.3 percent) are the least frequently reported problems (Table 4.19). Thus, it seems that if these incidents, other than vandalism and user property damage, are associated with park users' perceptions or behavior, this sample would be adequate to reveal the nature of such associations. Instead, the weak and, in the case of perceptions of safety, contradictory relationships exhibited suggest that intervening, or more influential, factors exist to explain perceptions of safety and avoidance behavior. It is interesting to note that in spite of so many park users reporting having witnessed various types of depreciative behavior in the park, it is still considered as safe, or safer, than downtown, neighborhood, and other park settings (Table 4.2). This appears to imply that a fairly wide range of behaviors are considered normal, if not acceptable, in many public settings and that witnessing such behavior in parks is not considered unusual. Reasons for Avoidance Behavior Some additional insights into park users' safety-related perceptions and behavior are provided by inspection of the responses to two open-ended survey questions. When asked why they avoided the park or areas within the park, the most commonly cited reason was "because of the kinds of people there" (24.5 percent of all reasons given). "Not safe for some people or for people alone” is the second most frequently mentioned reason, followed by concerns about people hiding in the woods or bushes (Table 4.20). Only one person mentioned avoiding areas where they had seen problems occur. 105 TABLE 4.20 REASONS FOR AVOIDING THE PARK OR PARK AREAS* Avoidance during: Reason Daytime Evening T§EE% N(%) N(%) ° Because of the kinds of people there 13 (33.3) 21 (21.0) 34 (24.5) Not safe for some peOple or for people alone 10 (25.7) 19 (19.0) 29 (20.8) People can hide in the woods or bushes 4 (10.3) 19 (19.0) 23 (16.5) Evenings are not safe 2 ( 5.1) 16 (16.0) 18 (12.9) Few other people around 3 ( 7.7) 10 (10.0) 13 ( 9.4) Physical hazards (park structures, water, etc.) 5 (12.8) 3 ( 3.0) 8 ( 5.8) Not well lighted 0 ( 0.0) 8 ( 8.0) 8 ( 5.8) Never totally safe/ not a safe place 2 ( 5.1) 3 ( 3.0) 5 (3.6) Have seen problems there 0 ( 0.0) l ( 1.0) l (0.7) Total 39(100.0) 100(100.0) 139(100.0) * Respondents could mention more than one reason 106 Thus, although there are no significant associations between reported avoidance and perceived user homogeneity or rule compliance, the park social environment does appear to influence users' safety related perceptions and behavior. It may be the location or spatial clustering of the types of users considered threatening that influences other users' behavior. In terms of who these "threatening" users are, survey res- pondents were also asked to identify the kinds of people who cause prob- lems in the park. Nearly sixty percent named "young people," "kids," or "teenagers" (Table 4.21). The question of whether, in fact, young people are the "kinds of people" being avoided in the parks requires detailed, site specific, observational studies to answer. Such studies could also further illuminate the influence of the physical environment, in terms of vegetation, lighting, and physical hazards such as rivers or park structures, on users' perceptions and behavior. Summary of Acceptance or Rejection of Study Hypotheses Hypotheses and Subhypotheses . Accepted or ~Rejected* Al Park users will report feeling safe in the park. Accepted A2 Park users will perceive evenings to be less safe than daytime in the parks. Accepted A3 Park users will rate the park as equally or more safe than other parks, their own neighborhood, and downtown areas. Accepted Bl Park users' perceptions of park safety, in general, are related to their age, gender, and familiarity with the park. Bla Younger park users will feel safer in the park than will older users. Rejected Blb Male park users will feel safer in the park than will female users. Accepted 107 TABLE 4.21 WHO CAUSES PROBLEMS IN THE PARK?* Eggup_ Nuubgg_ Percent Kids, teens, or young people ' 70 59.3 Drinkers or drug users 24 20.3 A few trouble-makers 20 16.9 Members of a particular ethnic group 5 4.2 Total 118 100.0 * Respondents could identify more than one group 108 Blc Park users who are more familiar with the park will feel safer than those who are less familiar with the park. Rejected 82 Park users perceiving other park users to be similar to themselves will feel safer than those who perceive other users as different from themselves. Rejected B3 Park users who report having been victimized or having witnessed behavior problems in the park or those who report knowing people who have, will report feeling less safe than those who have not had these eXperiences. Rejected B4 Park users who report feeling less safe in the park will also report more avoidance behavior than those users who report higher levels of perceived safety. Accepted B5 Park users' age, gender, and familiarity with the park are related, in general, to their reported avoidance of some park areas due to safety concerns. BSa Younger park users will be less likely to report avoiding some park areas than will older users. Rejected 85b Male park users will be less likely to report avoiding some park areas than will female users. Accepted BSc Park users who are more familiar with the park will be less likely to report avoiding some park areas than those who are less familiar with the park. Rejected B6 Park users who are more familiar with the park will be less likely to report avoiding some park areas than those who perceive other users as different from themselves. Rejected B7 Park users who report having witnessed behavior problems in the park, or those who report knowing someone who has been victimized there, will report more avoidance behavior than those who have not had these experiences. Rejected * Acceptance or rejection of hypotheses is based on statistical significance (equal or greater than .05) of Kendall's Tau, the strength of association as measured by Kendall's Tau (greater than or equal to .30), and the proportional reduction in error as measured by the Asymmetric Uncertainty Coefficient (greater than or equal to .10). CHAPTER FIVE: SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS In the first portion of this chapter the specific results of this study are summarized and discussed. Next, the theoretical implica- tions of these results are examined in the context of both behavioral geography and leisure studies. In the concluding portion, the practical implications and recommendations for park managers, planners, and law enforcement are discussed. Summary and Discussion of Results Relative Park Safety The majority of people using the three study parks do appear to regard them as safe environments, at least during the day. In fact, similar to the campers in Clark, Hendee, and Campbell's study, many park visitors regard the parks as safe and orderly places in spite of their reports of experiencing or knowing about criminal or depreciative beha- vior in these settings [80,83,84]. Further, a lower proportion of these park users report safety concerns than do Malt Associates' urban park user respondents [119]. However, the format of the questions evaluating safety concerns is not identical in these two studies and Malt Associates did not distinguish between day and evening park use, although they re- mark that their respondents "do not think in terms of evening usage" [119]. 109 110 There is clearly a difference in perceived safety between day and evening park use among this study's respondents. Although nearly sixty percent of the park users questioned reported feeling safe in the park during the evening, this is a considerably smaller proportion than the ninety percent reporting feeling safe alone there during the day. This is consistent with general "fear of crime" research and is likely due to overall differences in perceived safety between day and night rather than being specific to the park setting. However, it does suggest that "implicit theories" and social definitions of park environments have a temporal dimension. The study parks also appear to be considered fairly safe, relative to other public settings. The large majority of respondents consider the parks to be safer than "downtown," and as safe or more safe than other parks. A smaller majority regard the parks to be at least as safe as their own neighborhood. Over half of the respondents regard the study parks to be as safe as other parks they know about and only one fourth report avoiding any park due to safety concerns. This implies that parks, in general, are considered to be fairly safe environments. The overall patterns of responses to these items suggests a hierarchy of perceived safety in public settings ranging from the most familiar and socially homogeneous settings -- i.e. neighborhoods ~- to socially hetereogeneous and less fmailiar areas such as "downtown." Thus, "parks" as a particular type of public setting appear to fall near the "safe" end of this continuum. 111 Association of Park Users' Individual Characteristics with Safety Related Perceptions and Behavior Although the above results generally support the hypothesis that more socially homogeneous environments are perceived as safer than socially heterogeneous settings, park users' responses to direct questions regarding the parks' social homogeneity and informal social controls within each park are not statistically associated with either their eval- uations of personal safety or their reported avoidance behavior in the park. There are also no demonstrable differences in safety perceptions or reported avoidance behavior between white and non-white park users. Almost three-quarters of the park survey respondents believe that most, if not all, other users share their values and over half feel that peOpIe in the park will "watch out" for one another. Perhaps the general image of parks as safe environments is strong enough to overcome the reserva— tions of those respondents who do not share the majority view of other users' values and rule compliance. 1 It is also possible that the survey questions simply do not pro- vide adequate measures of perceived social homogeneity or informal social control. Possibly these judgements are not easily articulated or overtly recognized by park users and the direct questions may be too simplistic. The fact that 13 to 24 percent of the survey respondents did not answer these questions may indicate that question wording was ambiguous or inappr0priate [149, pp.lS7-l6l]. Gender is the personal characteristic most strongly associated with perceptions of park safety and reported avoidance behavior. Females have the highest fear index scores, indicating they would not feel safe alone in the park at any time. Less than one percent of the 112 male respondents report feeling unsafe in the park during either the day or evening. Males are somewhat more likely to report avoiding certain areas of the park due to safety concerns but females still comprise over 80 percent of all those reporting both day and evening avoidance behavior. This is consistent with the findings of both general fear of crime research and Malt Associates' study of perceptions of safety in urban parks [101,119]. Malt Associates report that the females in their sample appear to be reacting to a general, undefined sense of anxiety rather than to specific features of the park environment. These findings suggest that males and females may have markedly different perceptions of park safety. There is a weak but statistically significant association between age and perceptions of park safety with reports of feeling unsafe in- creasing with the respondent's age. However, only eight people over 55 years old were interviewed. A larger sample with correspondingly higher proportions of middle age and elderly respondents might provide a more reliable measure of the association between age and fear. Since less than ten percent of all the user groups observed in the parks included older people, it is tempting to speculate that older users may be avoid- ing the parks entirely due to concerns about safety. There may, however, simply be few attractions in these three parks for older users, the parks may be less convenient fer older visitors to get to than other parks, and/or there may simply be low proportions of older people in the park service areas. The fact that no association was found between age and reported avoidance behavior also does not support such speculations. Finally, as Malt Associates report, even young women report park safety 113 concerns, thus the influence of gender may mask or overwhelm existing associations between age and safety concerns. There are also weak but statistically significant associations between two measures of familiarity with the park and feelings of safety. The proportions of respondents reporting feeling unsafe in the parks increases with the distance between a respondent's residence and the park and decreases with the frequency with which he or she visits the park. Neither measure of familiarity is associated with reported avoidance behavior. The lack of a strong association between familiarity with the park and perceptions of safety or reported behavior may be due to the over- riding differences between males and females. It is possible that, as with the lack of demonstrable relationships between park social homo- geneity and perceptions of park safety, the image of parks in general or people's implicit theories about the safety of these particular ”types" of park settings influence their safety related perceptions and behavior more strongly than does their actual familiarity or experience with the particular park in question. Association of Exposure to Antisocial Behavior in the Parks and Safety Related Perceptions and Behavior The pre-eminence of ideas or implicit theories about the park environment in determining evaluations of park safety is also suggested by examining the relationship between respondents' exposure to crime and other depreciative behavior in the parks and their evaluations of park safety and reported avoidance. Over ninety percent of those report- ing feeling unsafe in the park during both day and evening have never been victims of serious crime or harassment in the park nor do they report 114 knowing anyone who has been. Conversely, only 7.1 percent of those reporting these incidents fall into the high "fear" category. In ad- dition, there are weak but statistically significant relationships between perceived safety and the frequency with which respondents report witnessing other users engaged in the potentially objectionable activ. ities of drinking alcohol and using drugs. These associations, however, are negative, indicating that people who report seeing this behavior frequently also tend to report feeling safe in the park. These findings are consistent with other studies which find little association between victimization and fear of crime [101, p.27]. The fact that witnessing potentially objectionable behavior in the park does not appear to be considered threatening is probably due to the fact that as respondents' age increases the frequency with which they report both witnessing all types of depreciative behavior and visiting the park decreases. Younger users may see more potentially objectionable or depreciative behavior simply because they spend more time in the park than do older users. Further, and perhaps more important, these be- haviors do not appear to be interpreted as personally threatening. In fact, younger users, in particular, may consider drinking alcohol, drug use, and rowdy behavior to be fairly acceptable and expectable types of park use. Although experience with serious crime or conflicts in the parks is not related to perceptions of safety, it is weakly but significantly associated with reported avoidance behavior. Two-thirds of the respon- dents who have either been victimized in the park or know someone who was report avoiding areas of the park or the whole park during the evening. Although there is no indication that these park users are 115 necessarily avoiding the particular area where they or someone they know was victimized, it does suggest that they may feel safe so long as they avoid certain areas or situations. Dubow, McCabe, and Kaplan report that at least some crime victims do appear to subsequently avoid the locations or types of areas where they were victimized [101, p.63]. There are also significant, although weak, positive associations between reported avoidance and the frequency of witnessing people lit— teringcnrvandalizing park facilities, being loud or rowdy, and violating traffic laws. This is consistent with conventional wisdom regarding the negative influence of litter, vandalism, and rowdiness. It also suggests that respondents were not thinking only of safety from serious crime when they answered these questions. Again, it cannot be establish- ed that respondents are, in fact, avoiding those areas where they have seen these behaviors. The relationships are weak and may simply be co- incidental. There may also be an underlying dimension of variation in park users' awareness or concern about depreciative behavior that makes some users more likely to report both theircnnIaVOidance behavior and other users' misbehavior. Overall, few people report being victimized or knowing victims of serious crimes or harassment in the parks. Most of the serious crime and harassment incidents reported are fights or verbal intimidation. However, about 75 percent of the respondents have seen some kind of potentially depreciative behavior -- mostly alcohol consumption, park rule and traffic or parking violations, and littering or rowdy behavior. Thus, although many park users are aware of these activities it does not appear to influence their assessments of the parks' social order since the large majority of the respondents report that other people in the 116 park are both well behaved and obey the rules. This suggests that implicit theories about these types of urban and suburban park environ- ments may include the expectation or acceptability of some types of potentially objectionable behaviors in these settings. Association of Perceptions of Safety with Avoidance There is a moderate to strong association between perceptions of park safety and reported avoidance behavior. People who do not feel safe alone in the park also tend to avoid visiting the park or areas within it, demonstrating a clear link between perception and behavior. More avoidance is reported during the evenings than during the day, rein- forcing the previous findings regarding the differences perceived to exist in park safety from day to evening hours. Not feeling safe alone in the park is a frequent reason given for avoiding the park or areas within it. This reveals an error in survey design. The avoidance questions were not intended to refer only to how the respondent did or would behave when alone. However, the placement of these questions after those asking about general perceptions of safety appears to have biased some respondents' answers to the avoidance ques- tions. This may have led to over reporting of avoidance behavior, in general, since few people actually visit the parks by themselves. It may also have masked more site specific behaviors by people responding that they simply would not visit the park alone, rather than reporting the particular areas of the park they might avoid when visiting the park with others.2 Almost one-fourth of those who report avoidance behavior do not report feeling unsafe alone in the park during either day or evening 117 hours. This, coupled with the finding that those respondents who report serious crime or harassment incidents appear to be more likely to report avoidance behavior than to indicate they do not feel safe alone in the park, again, sUggests that some peOple may feel safe so long as they avoid certain park areas or times of day. However, generalizations based on the responses of those few respondents who report experiencing criminal incidents are questionable. It is probably more important to note that 76 to 84 percent of those who report avoiding the park or areas in the park do not report exposure to serious crime or harassment in the park. Most peOple, then, appear to base their behavior on perceptions of re- lative safety, rather than previous experience with threatening events. Study Limitations Both the small number of park users interviewed and the few park sites included in the study limit the generalizability of the above find- ings. Although the park user sampling procedure insures a reasonably representative sample of users in the three study parks, these three parks may not be an adequately representative sample of district parks, in general. Further research defining the characteristics of district level parks and district park users is necessary to evaluate the general— izability of these findings in terms of their reflection of the "image" of this type of park setting. Low numbers of respondents also preclude separate analyses of different user grOups such as males versus females and older versus younger park users. Low numbers of respondents also may result in under representation of certain groups, thus missing important variations in park users' perceptions and behavior. 118 While the focus of the study is limited to questions of park users' safety related perception and behavior, a complete investigation of both the impact of crime on park use and the overall image of district parks requires a representative sample of both park users and potential park users. People presently using the park represent a self-selected group of the general population in terms of their behavior and views of park safety. Clearly, peOple who consider the parks to be very dangerous locations probably visit them very seldom, if at all. Determining the validity of survey measures of perceptions and behaviors is always difficult. Many relevant concepts and attitudes, such as views of the effectiveness of informal social controls in a given setting, may be difficult to adequately articulate for both the researcher and the respondent. Attempts to isolate one aspect of the environment and ascertain its relationship to reasonably complex con- structs such as perceptions of personal safety, may lead to over- simplification. Further, asking respondents whether or not they would feel safe alone in the park may be overly artificial since few peOple actually visit the parks alone. Responses to questions about hypotheti- cal situations, in general, may not provide reliable indications of actual behavior. Also, questions about generally "safe" or "unsafe" areas or times may not always elicit responses relevant specifically to perceiVed safety from criminal attack. Until better measures of these constructs can be developed and, where possible, validated by observational data, these items can only be judged in terms of face validity. Further work- in this area requires both replication of current measures with larger samples and investigation of alternate methods of gathering relevant information. 119 Future Research A full understanding of the interactions among the operational, perceptual, and behavioral environments of people using district parks in urban and suburban settings requires a much more detailed and smaller scale approach than was employed in this study. Data aggregation prevents identification of the specific combination of factors that may influence a particular users' response to various physical and social factors in the parks. A research approach employing detailed behavior mapping and iin-depth, unstructured interviews with different types of users would add some "meat" to the "bare bones" so far supplied. A more fine-grained analysis could explore how people move through the park and select activity locations. For example, the "kinds of people” being avoided could be more readily determined using this approach. Informal discussions revealed that in Potter Park both park users and park personnel identify the park- ing lot area as the site of many problems. During the fieldwork, inter- viewers noticed that it was not uncommon for a group of four to six young people to dominate an entire parking bay (Figure 3.1) by tossing a fris- bee down its center aisle, turning up their car stereos, and shouting back and forth with companions perched on nearby car hoods. Some older users and family groups reported being intimidated or angered by this behavior. They were reluctant to drive through or park in an area where this was occurring and were disturbed to find youngsters sitting on or lounging against their car when they returned to the parking lot from another part of the park. Both users and nearby residents also complained about the often incessant parade of young people "cruising" in their cars through the parking lot. Behavior mapping and unstructured interviews 120 could more readily demonstrate the perceptual and behavioral impacts of this type of-park behavior. Detailed observational studies could also explore the relationships between reported perceptions, reported behavior, and actual behavior. Both temporal and spatial variations in park use could be exmained. The results of this study suggest that females may avoid certain areas of the park, possibly wooded areas for example, and may avoid parks during the evening due to fear of crime. Observations could validate these findings and/or demonstrate other differences between male and female park users' spatial behavior. 2 Further investigation of differences among age groups in their behavioral expectations and responses to the park environment is also warranted. A larger survey sample of park users, including more older visitors, combined with observation of park behavior patterns, may reveal different social definitions of recreation places, based on age and peer group expectations, and different patterns of park use due to avoidance of "non-conforming" users as well as different interests and activities. A survey of local residents might reveal patterns of non-use of park areas attributable to fear of crime and non-conforming values and expec- tations about what kinds of behavior and users might be found in differ- ent types of parks. Clearly, full investigation of the relationships among safety related perceptions and behavior, individual character- istics, and types of park settings requires extensive household survey data. Future studies would benefit from better measures of objective risk in the parks. Careful records of the location, time, and details of criminal and depreciative behavior in the parks are needed. Gaining 121 these measures is, however, problematic. Both parks and law enforcement agencies are currently understaffed and personnel complain about the seemingly unending supply of paperwork and reports already required of them. Thus, it is doubtful that agencies will provide either the park coverage or report details needed. Park users' objectivity and willing- ness to supply information about these incidents is questionable. Fin- ally, serious or violent crimes in the parks appear to occur infrequent- ly, in general, and it may be difficult, if not impossible, to objective- ly define zones of risk within the park based on these events. A research agenda for future efforts would include, first, detailed case studies employing both interviews and observations in selected parks to better determine the appropriate dimensions of variation to be addressed. As these questions begin to be answered, more compre- hensive, general approaches, could be launched to improve the represent- ativeness and quality of the existing data base. Better understanding of how people use and perceive recreation settings and identification of those environmental elements that may be considered threatening has both theoretical and practical significance. Theoretical‘lmplications Recreation geOgraphy is concerned with the location, use, and physical, social, and economic impact of recreation places. An import- ant aspect Of recreation places is how social definitions and behavioral expectations abOut them are formed-and how these place "images" influence the use of these spaces. This in-depth look at one particular aspect of park user perceptions and behavior in a particular type of recreation setting adds to the understanding of how people perceive recreation places 122 and the "image" that people have of this type of district level parks By comparing people's fear of crime and avoidance behavior in parks to that in other public settings, an indication is provided of the generalizability of other studies of fear of crime. Additional informa- tion is supplied regarding how people use and perceive public space and respond to the threat of criminal behavior. In the broadest sense, this study contributes to the overall field of social geography by demonstrating, once again, the importance of place "images." Study results indicate that district parks tend to be considered as safe or safer than their surrounding urban and suburban contexts. This overall "image" appears to be as much or more influential in determining park users' evaluations of personal safety than their familiarity with the park, their perceptions of whether other users hold values similar to their own and obey the rules, and their own experience with criminal and other depreciative behavior in the park. Further, the differences demonstrated between male and female park users' perceptions and behavior, once again point out that males and fe- males, in many ways, may occupy different behavioral and perceptual en— vironments. These different "life worlds," particularly the environ- mental factors that constrain femaleS' spatial behavior, deserve further attention by geographers if the discipline is to provide relevant insights into real world behavior. The purpose of behavioral studies in geography is to increase our understanding of the interactions between humans and their environment. There is some disagreement regarding whether work in this area should be directed primarily toward gathering quantifiable information to improve predictive models of behavior and/or decision-making or whether it is 123 more apprOpriate to gain a qualitative understanding of how people feel about and operate within the environment. This study touches upon both concerns by identifying, measuring, and comparing the characteristics of a particular type of setting and the peOple using this environment to the behaviors and "images" elicited, in a consistent and replicable fa; shion while explicitly examining the link between perception and behavior. At the same time, the more humanist tradition within geography of seeking to understand the meaning and experiential qualities of the environment is not ignored. 2 Theory development in human geography and leisure studies must inevitably be based on a myriad of case studies. Extensive descriptive work is required to identify regularities before potential explanations can be formulated and tested. Whether the ultimate goal is to predict behavior or to simply understand it, overt behavior and the human per- ceptions and ideas that produce that behavior must be investigated with- in many different, unique environmental settings. The regularities and variations that emerge provide the essential building blocks for theories of human-environment interaction. This study provides one such building block by examining percep~ tions and behaviors related to a particular type of phenomena within the context of a particular type of setting. It replicates portions of earlier studies in different environments and is, itself, replicable. Further, by concentrating on park settings that are similar in terms of their position along the "recreation opportunity spectrum," it contributes to the development of theory within recreation geography and the inter- disciplinary field of leisure studies relating recreation behavior, ex- pectations, and preferences to specific types of recreation settings. 124 A central question in relating recreation behavior to recreation environments is the "question of how shared images of such places arise and ensure orderly group life" [73,p.7l]. Both Lee and Fridgen point out that different types of recreation settings elicit different expect- ations among users regarding the kinds of behavior that will be acceptable there and that different user grOups in the same park may have different images or "implicit theories" about that environment [70,73]. In turn, conflict may arise among users who do not share the same social defini- tion of the park. Much of the work in this area has concentrated on land use conflicts or depreciative behavior in rural park settings and with user groups such as wilderness backpackers, snowmobilers, crosscountry skiers, and campers. This study provides some information about social order in district level parks located in more urbanized settings. In terms of different user groups' differing social definitions of this type of park, there is some indication that younger park users may have somewhat different ideas about appropriate behavior in these types of park settings than those of older visitors. Survey responses indicate that younger users tend to visit the parks more frequently and are more likely to report witnessing a wider range of depreciative behavior in the park compared to older users. Yet, younger users are somewhat less likely to report feeling unsafe in the park than are the older users. The fact that the majority of survey respondents identified "young people" as the group most likely to cause park problems further suggests that youthful visitors may operate according to somewhat different behavior norms and expectations in the parks. This, however, may be true in many other public settings, as well. 125 This study confirms the preeminence of people's images or theories about environments in forming their perceptions of particular park envir- onments. Park users' responses to and perceptions of potential safety threats in park environments do appear to differ among park settings. Contrasting wilderness users' and campers' reported perceptions and behavior suggests that as social heterogeneity and the prevalence of "urban" values among setting participants increases, so does the accept- ability of formal behavior controls while the proportion of visitors using natural areas in these parks may decrease. It appears that as one moves through the recreation opportunity spectrum from campers to day users of district level urban and suburban parks, these differences be- come more marked. The use and peGCetions of wooded areas, in particular, appear to be affected. Rural park locations may have a "safe" image associated with small town nostalgia, wilderness ethic expectations, and perceived social homogeneity. Clark, Hendee, and Campbell note that the image of the campground as a safe and crime free environment is as pervasive as it is misleading [80]. Perhaps when people use more urban park settings, larger images of urban problems and behavior norms dominate their perceptions of the park. While urban parks are perceived to be less dangerous than downtown areas, they may be "tainted" by urban images of crime and social diversity. Symbolic expressions of formal control, including a "clean and neat" setting may be more important in parks in urbanized locations than in rural campgrounds to reassure participants about the predict- ability Of the social setting. This may be one reason witnessing litter and vandalism was associated with avoidance behavior in this study. Still, seeing depreciative behavior in the parks does not appear to bother many 126 of the park users in this study. This implies that either users have considerable faith in formal and informal means of social control to keep this behavior at acceptable levels or they do not perceive these types of behavior as having an impact on their personal use of the park. It is important to study these settings closely and identify just what mechanisms are at work in maintaining social order. More detailed studies of the influence of depreciative behavior on park users' perceptions and behavior may assist in identifying the types and levels of such behavior that are considered acceptable by different user groups: It is also important to identify how the specific elements of the park environment combine to influence both perceptions of safety and_objective risk. For example, this study suggests that groups of young people may be considered threatening in the parks, par- ticularly by women and older users. Future research could evaluate whether it is the absolute number of young people, the number and size of groups of young people, the location or relative concentration of these groups, or the types of behavior they are engaging in which in- fluences whether they are perceived as threatening or not. An interest- ing and related avenue for future work would be the investigation of young people's recreation goals, needs, and preferences to facilitate their expression in ways or locations that will not have an adverse effect on other park users. Studies of crime and perceptions of crime in urban parks suggest that parks and areas within parks that attract large numbers of visitors experience the most problems. More research is needed to evaluate whether this is due to increased numbers of potential offenders and/or victims, social diversity, the difficulty of providing adequate supervision 127 for large numbers of people, certain aspects of the park settings such as designs which do not facilitate informal controls or surveillance or attractions which provide certain criminal opportunities, or to some identifiable combination of these factors. Moreover, the study of safety related perceptions and behavior is an important component in the larger pursuit of identifying those elements of leisure environments that contribute to or detract from setting participants' satisfaction. The findings of this study, for example, suggest that males and females may view recreation settings differently and have somewhat different site specific recreation preferences, needs, and behavior. Further, the importance of temporal variations in park use and perception requires fUrther attention. Implications and Recommendations for Park Planning and Management If social controls in the park are to "be designed to fit the moral order of specific user groups," then decisionmakers must identify the moral order or behavior norms of the relevant group or groups and then decide which group's normative order is to be preeminent in program and facility design [73,p.82]. Several studies have demonstrated that recreation management approaches based on the managers' rather than the users' values may have unanticipated or ineffective results [75,80,125, 129]. For example, both Clark, Hendee, and Campbell's study of campers and this study or urban and suburban park users suggests that some dep- reciative behavior may be "deviance by definition" [80.83,84]. That is, park management may prohibit or discourage certain types of behavior that users do not perceive as detrimental to their recreation experience. In some cases unpopular rules or management practices may be necessary 128 for resource protection. In other instances, rules based on managers' perceptions of what park users want or need may be at odds with the actual desires of many users. More to the point, rules that many park users do not agree with are likely to be violated. Alcohol consumption, for example, is illegal in Potter Park, yet nearly half the survey respondents in Potter report having seen people drink alcohol there and 28 percent report seeing this behavior "often." Unenforceable rules, from the practical point of view, may also do more harm than good in parks. Different user groups may require different types of enforcement controls. While campers and backcountry users appear to respond well to "appeals to help" in litter control efforts, for example, more formal and punitive approaches may be required with other groups or more serious violations [73]. Available research suggests that adolescents and members of "deviant" subcultures may be particularly unresponsive“ to the types of infOrmal social controls that tend to govern predomin- antly middle class, family oriented, leisure settings. Visible and effective formal controls and/or physically ejecting or spatially segre- gating nonconforming user groups may be the only solution. Effectively barring nonconforming user groups, or strictly regulating their behavior, may not, however, always be the best solution for park conflict problems. In many cases, these individuals may be pursuing legitimate recreation goals. The young people cruising the Potter Park parking lot, for example, were seeking diversion, peer group interaction, and perhaps stimulation and challenge. These are all goals that recreation professionals and park agencies are pledged 129 to support and facilitate. Eliminating problems by eliminating problem makers often only shifts the location of the problem behavior within the larger park agency or community framework. A more effective, albeit difficult, solution would be to identify alternate behaviors and/or locations to satisfy the needs of these park users. Moreover, the issue of problems relating to young people congregating in parks is part of the larger dilemma of how our society chooses to deal with its young peOple who often have no jobs, few responsibilities, little parental supervision, and considerable mobility. More research is needed to identify the effectiveness of various control strategies in different types of recreation settings. Research to date suggests that formal controls may be more acceptable and ef- fective in socially heterogeneous leisure settings than in socially homogeneous sites where infOrmal social controls are recognized by most participants. The nature and expression of both formal and infor- mal controls, too, can vary considerably, Public reaction to the mounted police patrol in Potter Park was considerably more favorable than that receivedlnrthe officers in patrol cars. Other research sug- gest that highly visible, "ranger" type enforcement personnel are more positively perceived, in general, than are officers with more standard "police" type appearance. Further, many people in the study parks re- marked that they would prefer to see more foot and horse patrols and less car patrol in the parks. Park agencies and local governments often cannot afford to dras- tically increase the number of enforcement officers in the parks or re-equip them. Some agencies have increased the level of surveillance in the parks by increasing the number of nonenforcement personnel or 130 instituting volunteer "Clean-up," "host," or "interpreter/ranger," programs. Many feel that these programs provide a low cost method of providing a visible Official "presence" in the park and hope they may foster feelings of personal responsibility for the park among site users. Enforcement agencies and divisions, however, are not uniformly enthusiastic about either changing from a "police" to a "ranger" image or encouraging volunteer patrol programs. Dilution of the symbols of police authority, they argue, can make dealing with trouble-makers in parks more difficult. The ranger's enforcement powers and public duties are perceived by many as different from those of police officers. Many officers also fear that volunteer groups with surveillance responsibil- ities can too easily get into dangerous situations that they have neither the expertise nor equipment to handle. Others warn that volun- teer patrols can become uncomfortably similar to vigilante groups. Another practical application of research in this area lies in identifying the elements of site design that may facilitate apprOpriate behavior and/or discourage inappropriate activities. User groups with different recreation goals, preferences, or behaviors might be en- couraged to use different parks or different areas within a park through either management or design. Some of the problems in Potter Park, for example, might have been avoided through a parking lot design and en- forcement or management policy that discouraged "cruising"znulmade staying in the parking area less attractive, encouraged young users to congregate in areas that family groups were not forced to pass through, and provided opportunities for park personnel to maintain a more constant, 131 friendly but watchful eye on their activities. In fact, aesthetically attractive areas where young People can park, cruise, and "hang out" are not necessarily a problem if they are adequately sdpervised and buffered from other park areas, residences, and transportation arteries. The overall influence of the physical park landscape on users' perceptions and behavior should also be considered. The results of this study suggest that peOple using urban and suburban parks may avoid using wooded or densely vegetated areas. A Wholesale clearing of vegetative screening in parks, however, is hardly desirable. Selective clearing of underbrush may be appropriate in some areas, particularly those adjacent to parking areas and pathways. The aesthetic value of natural vegeta- tion is essential to most parks' attractiveness and "image." The des- irability of clearing vegetated areas depends on what kinds of activities management wishes to encourage or discourage. Areas designated as ecologic preserves or "natural areas" in urban and suburban parks might best be managed primarily for their aesthetic and educational value, neither expecting nor encouraging more general recreational use. Guided interpretive activities might be emphasized in extensive natural areas to increase their use by a larger proportion of potential users. Thus, people who might be afraid to venture into the woods alone would be accompanied by an "authority figure." Regular patrol and guided walks will also discourage inappropriate use of these areas. Park managers might be particularly alert to the safety cues in their facilities if they serve or wish to serve a large portion of the female population. This is an especially salient point in urban and 132 suburban parks where people may wish to visit for a fairly short period of time after work or school or on weekends. Female users might appreciate an area where they feel they can sunbathe or bring their children without fear of harassment. The increasing number of female-headed families makes safe recreation opportunities for women, alone or with other women and children, an important issue. Similarly, park agencies that wish to encourage evening use of their facilities will need to be aware of the elements that influence perceptions of safety during the evening hours. Visibility, lighting, use patterns, and social controls are likely to be particularly important to pot- ential evening users. Park activities may have to be supervised and transportation provided to lure older people and females, in particular, to the park after dark. Finally, as this study indicates, park agencies need to keep accurate records of the number and types of people using their facilities, activity patterns, depreciative behavior patterns, and enforcement activities. Accurate baseline data is essential to designing and eval- uating control and prevention programs. Experimental approaches to behavior control in parks requires accurate, longitudinal data to assess their effectiveness. Under tight budget conditions the cost effectiveness of enforcement activities requires documentation. All park agency personnel want to minimize both the human and monetary costs of criminal and other depreciative behavior as quickly and effect- ively as possible. Decisionmakers need accurate measurement of the apprOpriate "hard data," supplemented by thoughtful analysis and insight into the "softer," experiential character of each park and its user population to meet this goal. LI ST OF REFERENCES 1] 2] 3] 4] 5] 6] 7] 8] 9] REFERENCES William D. Pattison. The four traditions of geography. Journal of Geography 63:211-126, 1964. Paul W. English and Robert C. Mayfield, editors. Man, Spacg, and Environment. Oxford University Press, New York, 1972. Larry Grossman. Man-environment relationships in anthropology and geography. Annals of the Association of American Geogrgphers 67(1):126-144, 1977. ’”' David Herbert. Urban Geography. Praeger Publis ers, New York, pp. 199-237, 1972. Patrick Lavery, editor. Recreational Geography. John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1974. Reginald Golledge and Gerard Rushton, editors. Spatial Choice and Spatial Behavior: Geogrpphic Essay§ on the Analysis of Preferences and Perceppions. Ohio State University Press, Columbus, Ohio, 1976. Daniel E. Georges. The Geography of Crime and Violence: A Spatial and Ecological Perspective. Association of American Geographers, Washington, D.C., 1978. S.S. Alfano and A.W. Magill, editors. Vandalism and Outdoor Recreation: Symposium Proceedings. U. S. D. A. Forest Service, Pacific Southwest Forest and Range Experiment Station, Berkeley, Ca.1976. Comptroller General of the United States. Crime in Federal Recreation Areas -- A Serious Problem Needing Con ressional and Aggncy Action. U. General Accounting Office, Washington, D.C., 1977. 133 10] 11] 12] 13] 14] 15] 16] l7] 18] 19] 20] 134 Kristina Goodrich. ' Skinny dipping: The old swimming hole breeds controversy. Parks and Recreation 15(5):33-37, 81, May 1980. Theresa N. Westover, Theodore B. Flickinger, and Michael Chubb. Crime and law enforcement. Parks and Recreation lS(8):29-33, Aug. 1980. Lawrence C. Hadley. Perspectives on law enforcement in recreation areas. In E.vH. Larson, editor, Recreation Symposium Proceedings, pp. 156-160. U.S.D.A. Forest Service, Northeastern FOrest Experiment Station, Upper Darby, Pa., 1971. Sheryl James. The cause of death. Lansing Maggzine, pp. 31-35, Dec. 1980. Jon Driessen. Problems in Managing Forest Recreation Facilities: A Survey of Field Personnel. U.S.D.A. Forest Service, Equipment Development Center, Missoula, Montana, 1978. Diana R. Dunn. Urban recreation research: An overview. Leisure Sciences 3(1):25 -57, 1980. National Park Service. Ranger Activities and Protection Division Report (mimeographed). Washington, D.C., 1979. Sacramento Union. Park vandalism. P. B-l, August 31, 1978. Bureau of Outdoor Recreation, Lake Central Region and National Park Service. National Urban Recreation Study. Chicagp/Gary. U. S. Government Printing Office, Region B, Denver, Co. 1977. Robert E. Park, Ernest W. Burgess, and Roderick D. MOKenzie. The City. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 111., 1925. William Michelson. Man and His Urban Environment: A Sociolog;gal Approach (with revision5)'. . Addison-Wesley Co., Reading, 1976. 21] 22] 23] 24] 25] 26] 27] 28] 29] 30] 31] 135 David Lowenthal. Geography, experience, and imagination: Towards a geographical epistemology. In English and Mayfield, eds. , Op. cit. 2, pp. 219-244. Originally published in Annals of the Association of American Geographers 51(3):24l-260, 1961. David Lowenthal and Hugh C. Prince. English landscape tastes. In English and Mayfield, eds., op. cit. 2, pp. 81-112. Orignially published in Gepggpphical Review 55:186-222, 1965. David Lowenthal. Environmental Assessment: A Comparative Analysis of Four Cities. American Geographical Society, New York, 1972. David Lowenthal. The American scene. Gepgraphical Review 58:61-88, 1968. Kenneth Thompson. Insalubrious California: Perception and reality.. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 59:50-64, 1969. Clarence Glacken. Traces on the Rhodian Shore. University of California Press, Berkeley, Ca., 1967. Thomas F. Saarinen. Environmental Planning: Perception and Behavior. Houghton Mifflin Co., Boston, 1976. Derk DeJonge. Images of urban areas: Their structure of psychological foundations. Journal of the American Institute of Planners 28:266-276, 1962. D.L. Linton. The assessment of scenery as a natural resource. Scottish Geoggaphical Magazine 84:219-238, 1968. K.D. Fines. Landscape evaluation: A research project in East Sussex. Regional Studies 2:51-55, 1968. Shue Took Wong. Perception of Choice and Factors Affectingy_ndustrial Water Supply Decisions in Northeastern Illinois. Univer51ty of Chicago, Department of Geography Resource Paper No. 117, Chicago, 1969. 32] 33] 34]' 35] 36] 37] 38] 39] 40] 41] 42] 136 Julian Wolpert. . . The decision process of spatial context. Annals of the AsSociation of American Geographers 54:537-558, 1965. Julian Wolpert. - Behavioral aspects of the decision to migrate. Papers and Proceedings of the Regional Science Association 15:159-169, 1965. Roger Downs and David Stea, eds. Impgu and Environment: Cognitive Mapping and Spatial Behavior. Aldine Publi§hing Co., Chicago, 1973. Peter Gould. Acquiring spatial information. Economic Geogrgphy 51:87-99, 1975. Gary Meore and Reginald Golledge, eds. Environmental Knowin : Theories, Researchyyand Methods. Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, Stroudsburg, Pa., 19762 Roger Downs and David Stea. Maps in Minds: Reflections on Cognitive Mapping. Harper and Row, New York, 1977.71— Marvin W. Mikesell. Landscape. In English and Mayfield, eds., op. cit. 2, pp. 9-15. Originally published in David L. Sill, ed., International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences 8:575-80, 1968. Marvin W. Mikesell. Tradition and innovation in cultural geography. Annals of the Association of American Gppgraphers 68:1-16, 1978. D.J. Walmsley. Positivism and phenomenlogy in human geography. Canadian Geogrgpher 28:95-107, 1974. Edward Relph. An inquiry into the relations between phenomenology and geography. Canadian Geographer 14:193-201, 1970. D.C. Mercer and J.M. Powell. Phenomenology and Related Non-Positivistic Veiwpoints in the Social Sciences. Department of Geography, MOnash University, Melbourne, Australia, 1972. 43] 44] 45] 46] 47] 48] 49] so] 51] 52] 53] 137 Trudi-E. Bunting and Leonard Guelke. Behavioral and perception geography: A critical appraisal. Annals of the Association of American Geogrgphers 69(3): 448-462, 1979. Gerard Rushton. On behavioral and perceptual geography. Annals of the Association of American Geo ographers 69(3): 463-464, 1979. Thomas F. Saarinen. Commentary-critique of Bunting-Guelke paper. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 69(3): 464-468, 1979. ‘ Roger M. Downs. Critical appraisal or determined phiIOSOphical skepticism? Annals of the Association of American Geographers 69(3): 468-471, 1979. Joseph Sonnenfeld. Geography, perception, and the behavioral environment. In English and Mayfield, eds., Op. cit. 2, pp. 244-251. Originally presented to the American Association for the Advancement of Science, December 27, 1968. Robert Sommer. Personal Space: The Behavioral Basis of Design. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, 1969. Robert Sommer. Depigu Awareness. Rinehart Press, Corte Madera, Ca., 1972. Kevin Lynch. The Imagg_of the City. M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1960. J. Douglas Porteous. Environment and Behavior: Planning_and Everday Urban Life. Addison-Wesley Publishing Co. ,Reading, Mass. 1977. Allan W. Wicker. An Introduction to Ecological Psychology Brooks/Cole Publishing Co., MOntery, Ca., 1979. Yi- Fu Than. Sign and metaphor. Annals of the Association Of American Geographers 68(3): 363-372,1978. s4] 55] 56] 57] S8] 59] 60] 61] 62] 63] 64] 138 George H. Stankey. Recreational geography: Its evolution and application to problems of wilderness management. . - In Harold A. Winters and Marjorie Winters, eds.;“Applications of Geographic Research: Viewpoints from Michigan’State Univer- sity, Department of Geography, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Mi., 1977, pp. 75-88. R.I. Wolfe. Perspective on outdoor recreation: A bibliographic survey. Geographical Review 54(2): 203-238, 1964. Michael Chubb and Holly Chubb. One Third of Our Time: An Introduction to Recreation Behavior, Resources, and Programs. John Wiley, New York, 1981. Jack B. Ellis and Carlton S. VanDoren. A comparative evaluation of gravity and system theory models for statewide recreational travel flows. Journal of Regional Science 6(2):S7-69, 1966. Charles S. Tapiero. The demand and utilization of recreational facilities: A probability model. ngional and Urban Economics 4:173-185, 1974. Stanley R. Lieber. Attitudes and revealed behavior: A case study. Professional Geographer 29(1):53-S8, 1977. Ervin H. Zube, Robert 0. Brush and Julius Gy. Fabos, eds. Landscape Assessment: Values, Perceptions, and Resources. Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, Community Development Series, Vol. 11, Stroudsburg, Pa., 1975. A.S. Mills, L.C. Merriam, Jr., and C.E. Ramsey. Public Opinion and Park Development. Agricultural Experiment Station Bulletin 516, Forestry Series 22, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, 1976. Dean MacCannell. The Tourist: A Neerheorygof the Leisure Class. SchoEken Books, New York, 1976. Kevin C. Kearns. On the nature and origin of parks in urban areas. Professional Geographer 20(3):l67—l70, 1968. Nicholas Helburn. The wildness continuum. ' Professional Geographer 29(4):333-337, 1977. 65] 66] 67] 68] 71] 72] 73] 74] 75] 139 Yi- Fu Tuan. Topophilia: A Study of Environmental Perception, Attitudes, and Values. Prentice -Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, 1974. Yi-Fu Tuan. Landscapes of Fear. Pantheon Books, New York, 1979. Linda Graber. Wilderness as a Sacred Space. Association ofiAmerican Geographers, Washington, D.C., 1976. Lyn H. Lofland. A World of Strapgers: Order and Action in Urban PublicpSpace. Basic BoOks, New York, 1973. Erving Goffman. The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Doubleday Anchor BoOks, Garden City, New York, 1959. Joseph D. Fridgen. Environment-behavior research: Implications for the study of leisure and recreation behavior. In Seppo E. Iso-Ahola, ed., Social Psycholpgical Perspectives on Leisure and Recreation, pp.357-389. Charles C. Thomas, Springfield, Ill., 1980. deleted Stephen and Rachel Kaplan. Humanscape: Environments for People.' Dquury Press, North Scituate, Mass., 1978. Robert G. Lee. The social definition of outdoor recreational places. In William R. Burch, Neil H. Cheek, Jr. and Lee Taylor, eds., Social Behavior, Natural Resources, and the Environment, pp. 68-84, Harper and Row, New York, 1972. Roger N. Clark and George H. Stankey. The Recreation Opportunity Spectrum: A Framework fOr Planning, Management, and—Research. U. S D. A. Forest Service, Pacific Northwest Forest and Range Experiment Station, General Technical Report PNW-98, Portland, Oregon, 1979. John C. Hendee, W.R. Catton, L.D. Marlowe, and C.F. Brockman. Wilderness Users in the Pacific Northwest: Their Characteristics, Values, and'Mana ement Preferences“ Pacific Northwest Forest and Range ExPeriment Station, U.S.D.A. Forest Service, Research Paper PNW-61, Portland, Oregon, 1968. 76] 77] 78] 79] 80] 81] 82] 83] 84] 85] 140 Robert C. Lucas. Wilderness perception and use: The example of the Boundary Waters canoe area. Natural Resources Journal 3:394-411,‘l963. George H. Stankey. Visitor Perception of Wilderness Recreation Carrying Capacity. U.S.D.A. Forest Service, Intermountain Forest and Range EXperiment Station, Ogden, Utah, 1973. » Betty G. Rossman and 2. Joseph Ulehla. Psychological reward values associated with wilderness use. Environment and Behavior 9(1):41-66, 1977. Robert G. Lee. Alone with others: The paradox of privacy in wilderness. Leisure Sciences 1:3-20, 1977. Roger N. Clark, John C. Hendee, and Frederick Campbell. Values, behavior, and conflict in modern camping culture. Journal of Leisure Research 3(3):143—159, 1971. Joachim F. Wohlwill and Harry Heft. A comparative study of user attitudes towards development and facilities in two contrasting natural recreation areas. Journal of Leisure Research 9(4):264-280, 1977. Russell J. Foster and Edgar L. Jackson. Factors associated with camping satisfaction in Alberta Provincial Park campgrounds. Journal of Leisure Research 11(4):292~306, 1979. Frederick L. Campbell, John C. Hendee, and Roger Clark. Law and order in public parks. Parks and Recreation 3:28—31, 51-55, 1968. Also in Clayne R. Jensen and Clark T. Thorstenson, eds., Issues in Outdoor Recreation,pp. 137-144, Burgess Publishing Company, Minneapolis, Minn., 1972. Roger N. Clark. Depreciative Behavior in Forest Campgrounds: An Exploratory Study. U.S.D.A. Forest Service, Pacific Northwest Forest and Range Experiment Station, Research Note PNW-16l, Portland, Oregon, 1971. Daniel Stokols. The experience of crowding in primary and secondary environments. Environment and Behavior 8(1):49-86, 1976. -86] 87] 88] 89] 90] 91] 92] 93] 94] 95] 96] 97] 141 David Ley. ' ' r The Black Inner City as a Frontier Outpost: Images and Behavior of a North Philadelphia Neighborhood. Association of American Geographers, Washington, D.C., 1974. National Park Service. Special Study: The 1974 Summer Season, Gateray National Recreation Area. New York-New Jersey, Nov. 1975. California Department of the Youth Authority. Gang Violence Reduction ProjeCt:‘First Evaluation‘Rgport. Sacramento, Ca., 1978. Project for Public Spaces, Inc. Exxon Minipark: A Re-Desigp and Managgment Prpposal. New York, 1978. '7 Department of Parks and Recreation. Why Go To The Park? Questions to Measure Parks, People, and Patterns: Rectip No. 10. ' State of California, The Resources Agency, Sacramento, Ca., 1979. Harold K. Cordell. Substitution between privately and publicly supplied urban recreational Open space. Journal of Leisure Research 8(3):160-l74, 1976. Clyde R. White. The relation of felonies to environmental factors in Indianapolis. Social Forces 10(4):498-513, 1931-32. Sidney J. Kaplan. Climatic factors and crime. Professional Geographer 12(6):1-4, 1960. Keith D. Harries. The Geography of Crime and Justice. McGraw-Hill Book Co., New York, 1974. Larry Stephenson. Spatial dispersion of intra-urban juvenile delinquency. Journal of Geogrgphy 73(3):20-26, 1974. Irving Hoch. Factors in urban crime. 6 Journal of Urban Economics 1:194-229, 1974. Harold M. Rose. The geography of despair. Annals of the Association of American Gepgraphers 68(4): 453-464, 1978. 98] 99] 100] 101] 102] 103] 104] 105] 106] 107] 108] 142 David Ley and Roman Cybriwsky. The spatial ecology of stripped cars. Environment and Behavior 6(1):53-68, 1974. David Ley and Roman Cybriwsky. Urban graffitti as territorial markers. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 64: 491-505, 1974. James Lawrence LeBeau.. . The spatial dynamics of rape: The San Deigo example. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1978. Fred DuBow, Edward McCabe, andeail Kaplan. Reactions to Crime: A Critical Review of the Literature. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice, Washington, D.C., 1979. Franklin D. Becker. The effect of physical and social factors on residents' sense of security in multi-family housing developments. Journal of Architectural Research 4:18-24, 1974. Lee Rainwater. Fear and house-as-haven in the lower class. Journal of the American Institute of Planners 32(1):23-37, 1968. Ian Burton and Robert W. Kates. The perception of natural hazards in resource management. Natural Resources Journal 3:412-441, 1964. Gilbert F. White and Eugene Haas. Assessment of Research on Natural Hazards. The M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1975. Ian Burton, Robert W. Kates, and Gilbert F. White. The Environment as Hazard. Oxford University Press, New York, 1978. A. L. Rydant. Adjustments to natural hazards: Factors affecting the adoption of crop-hail insurance. Professional Geographer 31(3):312-320, 1979. Ruth Shonle Cavan. Criminology. Thomas Y. Crowell Co., New York, 1962. 109] 110] 111] 112] 113] 114] 115] 116] 117] 118] 143 John J. Gibbs. Crime Against PerSons in urban, Suburban, and Rural Areas: A Comparative Analysis of Victimization Rates. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, U.S. Department of Justice, Washington D.C., 1979. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration. Criminal Victimization in the united States. U. S. Department of Justice, U. S. .Government Printing Office, Washington, D. C. 1976. San Diego: Public Attitudes About Crime. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, National Crime Survey Report NO. SD-NCS-C-SO, NCJ-4624S, U.S. Department of Justice, Washington, D.C., 1977. Cincinnati: Public Attitudes About Crime.* Law EnfOrcement Assistance Administration, National Crime Survey Report NO. SD-NCS-C-ZZ, U.S. Department of Justice, Washington D.C., 1979. Pittsburg: Public Attitudes About Crime. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, National Crime Survey Report No. SD-NCS-C-29, U.S. Department of Justice, Washington D.C., 1979. New Orleans: Public Attitudes About Crime. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, National Crime Survey Report No. SD-NCS-C-27, NCJ-46242, U.S. Department of Justice, Washington D.C., 1977. Miami: Public Attitudes About Crime. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, National Crime Survey Report No. SD-NCS-C- 24, NCJ- 46239, U. S. Department of Justice, Washington, D. C. 1977. Visiting California parks and campsites in the 80's Dateline: NRPA 3(1):l, 11, 1980. M. Joan McDermott. Criminal Victimization in Urban Schools. Law Enforcement Assistance Administration, U.S. Department of Justice, 1979. Paul F. Rasmussen. A study of crime in the parks of Chicago, Illinois. unpublished masters project, University of Illinois at Chicago Circle, 1978. 119] 120] 121] 122] 123] 124] 125] 126] 127] 144 Harold Lewis Malt Associates, Incorporated. An Analysis of Public Safety as Related to the Incidence of, Crime'in Parks and Recreation Areas in Central Cities. U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, Washington, D.C., 1972. Theresa Westover and Michael Chubb. Crime and conflict in urban recreation areas: Research in progress. Proceedings, Second COnference on Scientific Research in the National Parks. San Francisco, Ca., 26-30 November, 1979. James F. Watts. The criminal law enforcement authority of park rangers in prOprietary jurisdiction National Parks —- where is it? California Western Law Review 13:126-152, 1976. Edward F. Conners. Public safety in park and recreation settings. Parks and Recreation 11:20-21, 55; 1976. George F. Rengert. Burglary in Philadelphia: A critique on an opportunity structure model. Presented to the Association of American Geographers Annual Meeting, Salt Lake City, Utah, April 1977. C.R. Jeffrey. Crime Prevention through Environmental Design, Second Edition. Sage Publications, Beverly Hills, Ca., 1977. Harriet H. Christensen and Roger N. Clark. Understanding and controlling vandalism and other rule violations in urban recreation areas. In Proceediggs of the National Urban Forestry Conference, V.l, pp. 63-84. U.S.D.A. Forest Service, Washington, D.CT, Nov. 13-16, 1978. Genie Zakrzewski. Survey views park crime (letter to the editor). Parks and Recreation lS(lO):4, 1980. Thomas A. Enerva . A study prepared for the United States Forest Service for analysis, evaluation, and recommendation concerning the depreciative behavior problems at Black Canyon campground. San Diego, Ca., June 1979. 128] 129] 120] 131] 132] 133] 134] 135] 136] 137] 138] 145 David E. Gray. The un-hostile park. In David Gray and Donald A. Pelegrino, eds., Reflections on the Recreation and Park MOvement,pp. 235-238, W. C. Brown Co., Publishers, U.S.A., 1973. Robert Sommer and Franklin D. Becker. The old men in Plaza Park. In Gray and Pelegrino, eds., pp. 249-251, 0p. cit. 128. Originally published in Landscape Architecture, Jan. 1969. Randy L. Scott. . A psychometric test for assessing the attitudes of Northern California wilderness users. Unpublished masters thesis, Humboldt State University, Ca., 1976. P. White, G. Wall, and G. Priddle. . Anti-social behavior in Ontario Provincial parks. Recreation.Research Review 6(2):13-23, 1978. Timothy B. Knopp and John D. Tyger. A study of conflict in recreational land use: Snowmobiling vs. ski-touring. - Journal of Leisure Research 5:6-17, 1973. Singing Sands Almanac. National Park Service, Indiana Dunes National Lakeshore, Oct. 1979. K. Stocking. The dune chronicles: The story of paradise lost and paradise gained at Sleeping Bear National Lakeshore. Detroit News Magazine, Aug. 19, 1979. Archie D. McDonald and Robert J. NeWcomer. Differences in perception of a city park as a supportive or threatening environment. In Gray and Pelegrino, eds., pp. 256—261, op. cit. 128. Seymour M. Gold. Urban Recreation Planning: Lea and Febiger, Philadelphia, 1973. T.B. Flickinger. Crime and law enforcement in Ohio's state parks. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Ohio State University, 1976. Tim S. Bynum and Dan M. Puuri. Vicarious victimiZation: An exploration of the fear of crime. Journal of Crime-and Justice, forthcoming. 1v. LIBRRRIES N lllllWllllHllWl E55355 (36511 MICHIGAN STRTE UN IllW""H“HWUIIIIWI 31293010