J NWWWWWI 1— 31293 010 {133091 5;) 0 ABSTRACT AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF THE EFFECTS OF COMMUNICATION MEDIA ON SOURCE CREDIBILITY By Ray E. Weisenborn This study investigated the main effects of four communication media (printed message, oral, visual, and com- bination) on terminal source credibility. The study also attempted to discover the additive nature of oral and visual media stimuli in the production of terminal source credibil- ity. Hypotheses tested were: (1) There is no difference between speaker images formulated for the same communicator employing either printed message, oral, visual, or combin- ation communication media. (2) Independently presented oral communication stimuli and visual communication stimuli pro— duce the same speaker image as when they are presented si- multaneously. A speech pretested for attitude neutrality was filmed by a speaker unknown to the experimental Ss. The speaker employed four different oral-physical delivery var- iations. Introduction of the speaker to all media groups was constant. Printed message 83 read the message and then Ray E. Weisenborn completed semantic differential source credibility scales. Other media Ss received their stimulus, read the message and completed the scales. Hypothesis one was rejected in the dynamism (p.‘=.0005) and authoritativeness (p.<:.025) source credi- bility dimensions. Results were not conclusive for the char- acter dimension because of a non-significant F-ratio (F = .453, p. <.715). To test hypothesis two, the effects of the printed message were removed from the remaining communication media. Summed differential dimension means for printed message were removed from corresponding dimensions in the other medias. The equation CMB-M = (O-M) + (V-M) was produced. This hypo- thesis was rejected in the dynamism (p. <.02) and authorita- tiveness (p. <.05) dimensions. Results of this study indicated: (1) There are dif- ferences in perceived credibility attributable to the media through which the message is presented. (2) In two source credibility dimensions oral and visual communication media stimuli-are interactive, not additive, in producing terminal source credibility. (3) The character dimension of source credibility, if relevant at all, has neither an additive or interactive effect on terminal source credibility. Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Speech and Theatre, College of Comunication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree. 55 5.5 5 Wector of Thesis // Guidance Comittee: flflé , Chairman /.‘ " L‘AXL.‘ “Q. s 4.. AM-ll in -’. V’)/( I. ‘ CUM AN EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF THE EFFECTS OF COMMUNICATION MEDIA ON SOURCE CREDIBILITY BY I, ix Ray El Weisenborn A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Speech and Theatre 1968 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The generation and completion of this writing rests on many shoulders: to Dr. James McCroskey's guidance and encouragement, to Drs. Jerry Anderson, Thomas Conner, David Ralph and Gordon Thomas' assistance, to the patience of my colleagues in graduate school, especially Charles Ertle and James Gardiner, and most of all, to my wife Donna, for two years of gui- dance, encouragement, assistance and patience. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ii LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii Chapter H I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Genesis of the Research Problem . . . . . . 3 Review of the Literature . . . . . . . . . 5 Ethos or Credibility of the Source 6 Nonverbal Communication of the Source . . . . . . . . . Preliminary Studies . . . . . . Preliminary Study One . . . Experimental procedure . Results and implications Preliminary Study Two . . . Experimental procedure . Results and implications Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . Hypotheses to be Investigated C C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O H 0‘ II 0 RESEARCH DESIGN 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 22 Hypotheses Investigated Operational Definitions Experimental Group . . . Test Instrument . . . . Subject Matter . . . . . Stimulus Preparation . . Stimulus Presentation . Data Collection and Analy l .. . .. . .. .. . .. . .. o o o o o ‘0 O o .. . .. . .. .. . .. . .. .. . .. . .. a: 01 SS iii Chapter III. REPORTING OF RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . Effects of Media on Source Credibility . The Additive Nature of Oral and Visual Communication Stimuli . . . . . . . . Secondary Measures . . . . . . . . . . . IV. CONCLUSIONS, SUBJECTIVE OBSERVATIONS, IMPLICA- TIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH AND SUMMARY . . Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Experimental Group Composition . . . . . Analysis of Media Effects Dynamism . . . . . . . Authoritativeness . . . Conclusions . . . . . . Analysis of Oral-Visual Communication Media Stimuli Additivity . . . . . Potential Confounding Variables to be Considered . . . . . . . Subjective Observations . . . . . Implications for Future Research Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . REFERENCES 0 O O O O O O I O O O O O O O I I O O O O APPENDICES O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O VITA O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 iv Page 32 32 35 38 40 40 40 42 42 43 43 44 44 49 51 55 59 73 Table 1. LIST OF TABLES Dynamism and Authoritativeness Semantic Differential Cell Comparisons and Results 0 O O O O O O O I O O O I O O O Dynamism, Character, and Authoritativeness Semantic Differential Mean Difference Scores and Results . . . . . . . . . . Dynamism, Character, and Authoritativeness Semantic Differential Means Adjusted to Constant Message Factor . . . . . . . . Chi-square Values and Probabilities for for Secondary Measures . . . . . . . . Page 34 36 37 39 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Type of Treatment Administered to Experimental and Control Groups . . . . . . 26 vi LIST OF APPENDICES APPENDIX Page A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL EXPLANATION . . . . . . 59 B SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL IN CREDIBILITY DIMENSIONS 0 O C O O O O O O I O O O O O O 60 C RESEARCH TEST BOOKLET . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 D INTRODUCTION OF EXPERIMENTAL SPEAKER . . . . 67 E ORAL-PHYSICAL DELIVERY STIMULI DESCRIPTIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 F ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE RESULTS . . . . . . . . 71 vii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The study of rhetoric has produced diverse informa- tion regarding the art of oral discourse. Scholars in the field have synthesized this material and attempted to in- struct peOple in the correct, or most effective, method of Speaking. Researchers have tested aspects of Aristotle's Rhetoric and proposed new theories about public speaking. This research has investigated such elements as organiza- tion, forms of support, reasoning, and the effect of the source on the impact of the message. In regard to this last area, concern has not been solely confined to public address research. The televised debates during the 1960 Presidential campaign stimulated numerous research studies. Many of these studies deal with public attitudes toward Nixon and Kennedy as they were per- ceived through the television media (Kraus, 1962). In al- most all instances the investigations at some time concerned themselves Specifically with the impact of the speaker's television image on viewers. The concept of ethos, or source credibility, cen- ters on the perceived speaker, However, most of the studies which have concerned themselves with persuasive discourse from the viewpoint of source credibility have neglected one of the fundamental aspects of the oral communication pro- cess. Studies have apparently not been concerned with many of the effects of a communicator's nonverbal communication. If we are to believe the conclusions of researchers who have worked with political oratory, the impact of the communicator's personality and visual appearance must be considered as primary factors (Rosenthal, 1964). In the post-analysis of the 1960 debates "the leaders of both par- ties seemed to believe that personality and image-making are the most powerful forces in swaying the electorate" (Kraus, 1962, p. 162). The late President Kennedy realized early in his career that he must be alert to the influence of tele- vision's visual image (Neubauer, 1967). Many persons felt that the turning point in Kennedy's race for the Presidency "came because of the favorable image he projected during his televised debates with Richard Nixon" (p. 89). "We wouldn't have had a chance without that gadget," Kennedy said. "It was television more than anything else that turned the tide" (p. 89). Research specialists like F. Clifton White, an advisor for men such as John Kennedy, discover and create discernable images for an individual. For a television appearance questions must be asked and answered. What camera angle suits him best? In what manner of dress should he be attired? What mannerisms should he use? White, as a participant on a television panel, stated that the image must be carried from the television screen to public campaign appearances (White, 1967). It is evident that attention is being directed toward nonverbal speaker images in the broadest sense by mass media and public relations research. However, for the most part, the studies provide semantic differential pro- file images of different political speakers which merely reveal which politician seems to create the most favorable image and do not tap the rational for the favorableness. The same appears to be true in almost all of the current image research. The assumption of this writer was, that the verbal and nonverbal stimuli of a communicator are interacting factors of importance in the production of source credibil- ity. Genesis of the Research Problem The image thesis of political oratory states that candidates and their agents create certain public images of a man, and, the candidate who casts the most favorable image gets the most favorable vote (Katz, 1966). Politi— cians recognize the importance of the image thesis. For instance, Senator Mark Hatfield (R-Ore.) has pointed out many facets of the image thesis which need to be considered by theorist and practioner. Is it the image the candidate is trying to establish through the image-building techniques of public rela- tions firms, promotional campaigns and publicity, and some of the other things that go with what we call pub- lic relations? Or, is it the impressions that people themselves gain from either a casual or uncontrived re- lationship they have had with the candidate? Not one the candidate has established, but one they have made seeing the candidate in a non-political environment making non-political remarks or comments. I think lots of things in politics may not be the factual as much as they may be the impressions that cause people to react in one way or another (1967). This writer had previously been interested in deter- mining the validity of the Senator's final inference. Prior to interviewing Senator Hatfield, the writer had conducted survey research in Oregon during August, 1967 (Weisenborn, 1967). This research investigated several basic aspects of the image thesis through the use of selected demographics and semantic differential means plotted in profile form. Summed semantic differential means revealed that there were differences, for instance, between Democrats' and Republi- cans' images of Hatfield. Senator Wayne Morse (D-Ore.) was included as a response stimuli in the survey. Data analysis showed that numerous demographic groups displayed signifi- cantly different attitudes toward the two men. This survey raised more questions than it answered. This writer became increasingly convinced that the image thesis was of considerable potential significance in the study of source credibility. However, a major question was generated by the research. What factors constitute the basic elements of the image thesis? In order to obtain information relevant to this question the writer surveyed the literature which seemed applicable to an investigation of speaker images. Review of the Literature Professional speakers rarely speak to audiences trained in speech criticism. Their hearers are usually laymen in this regard. This notion becomes quite meaning- ful when one realizes that in Brilhart's study (1967) lis- teners untrained in speech tended to evaluate speakers more accurately than their messages. That is, there was a sig- nificantly higher correlation between listener's ratings of several speaker's credibility than there was between ratings of message elements. Brilhart's findings warrant further investigation of the relationship between perceptual ten- dencies and the focus of listeners. Giedt (1958) reported data showing that personality ratings can be altered by raters noting such small manner- isms as averting eyes or touching the face. The personal- ities Giedt speaks of are those of image, and may not ne- cessarily reflect those of the true personality. Thus, an actor portraying a specific state of neurosis can be ana- lyzed more accurately than the neurotic simply because he will present a patterned series of traits for that condi- tion. The neurotic would not make such fine distinctions, i.e., his actions would be individualistic. Even though we may study words and movements, it is difficult to see what a person is really like (Giedt, 1958). Ethos or Credibility of the Source Personality may be difficult to assess, and in the same vein, so is the credibility of a communicator. Rosen- thal (1966) states that oral persuasion is that phenomenon which "involves a Speaker generating visual and aural cues which evoke neural and chemical responses from the listen- er" (p. 115). He also presents the following dichotomy of the persuasive act: When a speaker delivers a speech with the intent of in- fluencing the behavior of his auditors in a particular direction, we may conceive of the communication er s2 as presenting two distinct objects as potential oci of listener reaction: . . . Either the image or the mes- sage will ultimately emerge as the dominating agent of value activation for the listener and this will deter- mine the basic character of the persuasive act (p. 119% McCroskey (1968) divides ethos or source credibil- ity into three basic categories. Initial ethos is the image the audience has of a com- municator before the inception of communication. . . . Derived ethos is the image of the communicator produced in tfie minds of the respondents as a result of the com- munication, including content, presentation and the circumstances in which the communication occurs. ESE? minal ethos is the image the audience has of the com- municator at the completion of the communication and is produced by the interaction of initial and derived ethos (pp. 58-59). The dimensions McCroskey prescribes are similar to the types of credibility discussed by Berlo, Lemert and Mertz (1961). The terms "ethos", "speaker image", and "source credibility" all refer to the same concept. This writing shall employ the latter term in its discourse. Source credibility has at least two dimensions, perceived authoritativeness and character. Within these dimensions, appearance of the source can affect initial credibility in oral communication (McCroskey, 1967). McCroskey (1967) also states that since derived credibility is produced by the act of communicating, it seems probable that internal evidence would have a substan- tial effect upon it. In the same vein, it seems probable that different channels of communication would have a sub- stantial effect upon derived credibility. In the credibility study by Haiman (1949) signifi- cant differences between mean shifts of opinion were asso- ciated with who the speaker was thought to be. Haiman found that college groups tended to place a high premium on a speaker's competence and that the prestige of a speaker, as conveyed to a student audience through previous knowledge of the speaker's reputation and through the chairman's in— troduction, does influence the effectiveness of his per- suasion. It is known that vocal cues are important in the formation of speaker images (Winthrop, 1956 and Harms, 1960), but evidence regarding the value of visual percep- tions to source credibility is minimal. The total scope of source credibility is definitely an area which needs more investigation. Miller and Hewgill (1965), after doing re- search in this area, asserted "that further attention to the physical and vocal cues affecting audience reaction to a speaker is warranted" (p. 43).1 Research on the effects of delivery on source cre- dibility could remove all cognitive content from the aural message. This has been done in audiology research, but its effectiveness and application seem limited in the area of general speech research (Sherman, 1954). The main problem arising from utilization of such a methodology is that con- tent continuity is lacking as the technique is based on playing tape recorded speeches backwards. Another study (Davitz and Davitz, 1959) investigated the ability of Ss to communicate with content-free speech. By having their ex- perimental Ss orally project specific feelings by reciting portions of the alphabet, these researchers found that Ss could express non-coqnitive content with oral expression alone. The applicability of the study to the concept of source credibility is, however, somewhat limited. Research reported above highlights the importance of the visually perceived speaker and the Davitz and Davitz study was only concerned with content-free speech in terms of a communica- tor's orality. 1The reader is referred to Greenberg (1966) and King (1966). Nonverbal Communication of the Source The scientific investigation of observable human communication stimuli is known as the study of kinesics. Birdwhistle (1955) defines this science as: The systematic study of how human beings communicate by body movements, including facial expressions, shifts in posture, and formalized gestures (p. 11). Basic elements in the study of kinesics are kines, allokines and kinemes. Kines are the smallest movement positions of a part of the body. Allokines are the differ- ent ranges of position possible for the kinemic movements. Kinemes are the various classes of allokines. Although they are seemingly minute, allokinic movements can be very important. President Kennedy, for instance, would often extend his right arm, index finger extended, in a gesture that made him appear to dominate a conversation (Neubauer, 1967). With training in kinesics it is possible, by only hearing a person speak, to determine what simple movements he is making. Paradoxically, the average person is obli- vious to the extent to which he communicates nonverbally. Hildreth (1954) points out that many people who use body movements which contradict their actual words, reveal that, consciously or unconsciously, they do not believe what they say. 2The reader is referred to Reusch (1964). 10 In regard to speaker images, kinesics and the aural aspects of human communication are combined. This joining becomes the end result of a speaker's delivery. We must condition ourselves, as Aristotle did, to the fact that delivery does affect results, especially through its impact on attitudes and emotions. Thus, delivery cannot be disre- garded in nonverbal communication research.3 However, this writing is not concerned with the intracacies of aural com- munication; but rather, with the importance of the visually perceived speaker. The term "nonverbal" has connotations which differ from this writer's conception of silent communication. Kinesics speaks only of human physical actions, disregard- ing whether or not that stimulus is simultaneously produc- ing another communication stimulus, either of a primary or secondary nature. In other words, kinesics assumes the physical stimulus to be the singular primary stimulus while in fact, it may be a secondary, incidental stimulus. Throughout the remainder of this chapter, this writer will continue to review the literature in terms of the defined denotation of "nonverbal" communication. However, in set- ting forth the research hypotheses and presenting the de- sign, this writer will employ the term "visual" in the 3The reader is referred to Woolbert (1920), Jer- sild (1928), Ehrensberger (1960) and Nadeau (1964). ll sense normally attributed to "nonverbal" communication. Senator Hatfield (1967) has recognized the impor- tance of delivery and highlighted the need to analyze its physical aspects. He says: Can you divorce completely the so—called impressions and images from the substantive part of the candidate as to what he says? In other words, can you separate what he says from how he says it? He may say some- thing in a very clever way that has been devised for him by a ghostwriter, speechwriter, or public relations firm. But still, there must be, I believe, a certain analysis of what he is saying as well as how he is saying it, how he verbally, and physically, presents his message (1967). Interpretation of nonverbal cues, state Thomkins and Izard (1965), is based, to a large extent, on the na- ture of the judgement task and increased exposure of judges to nonverbal stimuli. Their series of socio-economic ex- periments were set up to probe the ability of untrained, unprompted judges to relate nonverbal cues to instances of spontaneous behavior. In one series of these experiments, judges matched, with 65 per cent accuracy, nonverbal cues to specific mental and physical states. Conversely, Giedt (1955) found in the prediction of patient responses to un- completed sentences that nonverbal cues resulted in a poor- er than chance correlation. That is, judges had a poorer than chance correlation when they matched a verbal or printed message stimuli to a visual stimulus. A general conclusion from the Thomkins and Izard (1965) research is that from still and motion pictures, 12 without any sound involved, judges cannot make accurate statements about true emotional states of interview Ss. Most information conveyed by still pictures alone is ambig- uous and judges find it difficult to assess a Ss' true per- sonality. However, when the test 53 presented pseudo-per- sonality traits, the judges could correctly identify them. Such an instance would be similar to the previously men- tioned one of an actor portraying a state of neurosis. In attempts to obtain personality ratings and to identify emotional states, Williams and Sundene (1965) dup— licated the Giedt methodology (1958). These experimenters implemented three basic stimuli: visual expression, vocal expression, and a combination of the two. Although their findings do not relate to this discussion, their experi- ments are mentioned because of their manner of testing. The use of verbal, nonverbal, and combination stimuli has also been implemented by Greenhill (1957) in communication research. Further discussion of this method of inducing experimental stimuli appears in the Preliminary Studies section of this chapter. There are many obstacles which must be overcome by researchers who wish to study the relationships between nonverbal communication and source credibility. Although Eckman (1964) states that there is no effective unit of measurement for kinesics, Krestinger (1962) has developed an electro-magnetic movement meter which reliably measures 13 gross bodily movement and levels of audience interest. The Krestinger meter is simply a recording device, and Weaver and Weaver (1965) specify problems remaining to be overcome. All of this suggests that, although vocal and visual cues have the capacity of generating meaning in the listener, the items of information cannot be expressed in bits. Moreover, this conclusion seems even more obvious for visual than vocal cues. Even if it were possible to record the visual postures and movements of the talking person and to use the probabilities of the occurrence of each signal as the basis of conver- sation into bits, the interpretation of these final bits would be difficult, if not impossible. Surely, many of these visual signals would be as vital as the words, and the remainder would lie on a continuum some- where between the two extremes. It is difficult to imagine a way to program a computer or anything else to do what the nervous system seems to do--to select only the relevant visual cues for compilation, or to record all movements and postures and weigh them according to their relatedness (p. 439). The studies cited above illustrate the problems connected with research involving picture cue stimuli. Basically, in terms of still pictures, a judge's rating correlations will increase with the increase of cue stimuli (Giedt, 1958). Research Seems to indicate that only with the use of movie film will more reliable nonverbal commun- ication research results be obtained. As Eckman (1964) states: In experiments [in] which nonverbal behavior was pre- sented as a communicative stimulus, and an observer's response to that stimulus was measured, still pictures, rather than motion pictures, eliminated any cues from sequence or movement patterns which are present to the observer (p. 301). All of the aforementioned studies represent just a beginning of the task of learning how important the oral, l4 and more specifically, the visual image of a speaker is to the communicative act. Research in the image-making of the political speaker and its importance has been noted. Stud- ies in the field of psychiatry indicate the importance of nonverbal personality research and current communication research has isolated and defined numerous factors of source credibility. To date however, this writer is aware of no research attempting to determine the relationship be- tween oral and visual communication stimuli as they relate to source credibility. Preliminary78tudies Previously mentioned research conducted by this writer led him to believe that one of the more significant problem areas in communication research needing further investigation was that of a source's nonverbal communica- tion. Lack of knowledge in this area led to preliminary research which asked: "Is there a difference between speaker images formulated by 'trained' judges receiving visual or oral communication stimuli? Is there a differ- ence between speaker images formulated by 'trained' and 'naive' judges receiving visual or oral communication stimuli?" Preliminary Study One To answer the first of these questions this writer 15 conducted experimental research at Michigan State Univer- sity in November, 1967. Ss were students enrolled in Com- munication 320, Nonverbal Communication. The null hypothe- sis to be tested was: "There is no difference between the speaker images of Ss sensitized to nonverbal communication theory who receive either oral or visual communication stimuli." The term "sensitized" was employed as the SS were in the process of learning more about nonverbal theory and could not be qualified as being "trained." However, as they had been exposed to more theory than a population with no background in nonverbal communication theory, the study equated them to trained SS. Speaker image was operation- ally defined as, "summed semantic differential means." Experimental procedure.--The experimental cells and stimuli in preliminary study one were: (1) Combination-- playing of sound movie film; (2) Visual—-playing of movie film without sound; (3) Oral--p1aying a tape recording of the movie film soundtrack; (4) Control--completion of the test instrument with no stimulus. These test stimuli were implemented on the basis of previously cited research by Williams and Sundene (1965), Giedt (1958), and Greenhill (1957). United States Senators from Oregon, Wayne Morse and Mark Hatfield, were the speakers presented in separate, three minute film clips. Testing procedure was identical for all cells. Af- ter being told they were being asked to participate in a 16 study developing a speech evaluation form and having a se- mantic differential explained to them (Appendix A), SS viewed the first film clip. Immediately afterwards they completed a semantic differential of eighteen source cre- dibility scale items (McCroskey, 1966b, Berlo, 25. 21., 1961, and Appendix B). This procedure was repeated for the second film clip. A factor analysis was obtained from the collected data and dimension means were obtained by summing across the scales in each dimension. Means were obtained on three dimensions for each of the stimuli. Results and implications.-—From the main hypothesis several sub-hypotheses were developed. Two null hypotheses were rejected at the .01 level of confidence. (1) It was found that there was a difference between the oral and Visual communication stimuli for Hatfield, and (2) that there were differences between the visual and combination communication stimuli for Morse. Preliminary study one in- dicated that there was a trend in the direction of differ- ences in speaker images of Ss sensitized to the theory of a specific type of communication media. The question then raised by this writer was whether or not the speaker images of judges trained in speech would be different from those of judges with no training in speech. Specifically, the question was: "Are there dif- ferences between all communication media stimuli speaker images formulated by trained and naive judges?" That is, 17 would the naive judges' image from a combination communica- tion media stimulus differ significantly from a trained judges' corresponding image, and would this also hold true for oral and visual communication media stimuli? Preliminaryistudy Two A second study was conducted in an attempt to an- swer the question generated by preliminary study one. The importance of the second study was the verification of the assumption that trained and naive judges' speaker images, derived from the same communication media stimuli, are sig- nificantly different. Experimental procedure.--Students enrolled in Mich— igan State University during Winter Term, 1968 were employed as experimental Ss in the second study. They were arbi- trarily placed into the same stimulus treatment cells im- plemented in preliminary study one. The SS were divided into trained and naive groups. The trained Ss (N = 172) were students enrolled in upper division speech courses who had completed a performance course in speech on the college level. The naive Ss (N=228 were students enrolled in dif- ferent sections of the basic speech performance course. Naive Ss participated in the research prior to any oral performance in the classroom. Stimulus presentation was identical to preliminary study one. Again, means were summed across the scales for each of the three credibility 18 dimensions. 2 tests for independent samples were run be- tween the different cells in each group. Stated as null hypotheses in equation form, the tests for each Senator were: Trained Naive combination = combination visual = visual oral = oral control = control Results and implications.--Between the first three of the above four hypotheses, there were no image differ- ences for Morse. The fourth hypothesis for Morse was sig- nificant at the .10 level. Only one test achieved signi- ficance at the .10 level for Hatfield, that being the vis- ual hypothesis. This writer's interpretation of the results of pre- liminary study two produced three assumptions. (1) Within both trained and naive groups, the varying degree of famil- iarity to the experimental speakers may have biased the results. The conclusion of the Haiman study (1949), which showed that ethos is affected by who the speaker was thought to be, were supported by preliminary study two. (2) By using two speakers in a single study, the effects of sequence interaction on respondents' images is difficult to control. Preliminary study two did not attempt to con- trol this variable and this factor may have biased the re- sults. (3) Message content may bias respondents' images. 19 The Hatfield message was presented in printed form to thir- teen Michigan State University freshmen and they were asked to complete a semantic differential after reading the mes- sage. The differential items were: like-dislike, agree- disagree, good-bad, honest-dishonest, informed-uninformed and valuable-worthless. Hatfield was not identified as the source of the message. The possible range of summed scores on the differential was 6-42, the hypothetical mean being 24. The mean for the test group was 28.6 with a variance of 1.87. In this instance message content produced a summed mean considerably above the presumed neutral point.4 While failing to reject its main null hypothesis, that there are no differences between trained and naive judges' images, the study raised additional questions. Is there a difference between images of a Speaker formulated by printed message, oral, visual, and combination communi- cation media stimuli? Conclusions The previously stated initial assumption of this writer was: "The verbal and nonverbal stimuli of a commun— 4McCroskey, et. al. (1968b), state: "In short, the assumption that the fiIdpSInt on individual bipolar scales represents the neutral point of attitude was demonstrated to be unacceptaBIe. . . . Thus, it appears that although the midpoint of a single bipolar may not represent the neutral point in attitude, the midpoint of the range of possible summated scores across Six bipolar scales does represent the neutral point in attitude" (p. 245). 20 icator are interacting factors of importance in the produc- tion of a source credibility." After conducting survey research, reviewing liter- ature pertinent to the study of speaker images and conduc- ting two preliminary experimental research investigations, this writer synthesized several conclusions regarding re- search in the area of Speaker images. These are: l. Listeners untrained in speech evaluated speakers more accurately than their messages. Physical movement is important in assessing Speak- er image. Speaker image is produced by both the heard and seen speaker. Appearance of the source can affect initial credi- bility. Prior knowledge about a communicator can affect speaker image. Judges need a continuum of stimuli, such as movie film, to evaluate the image of a communicator if that communicator is not observed live. Messages must be controlled to evaluate speaker images. In speaker image research it is not necessary to employ judges sensitized to specific types of com- munication media. In image research judges may be trained or naive 21 in speech. For all practical purposes, the terms ethos, source credibility, and speaker image are synonymous. From the writing above it is obvious that a great deal is known about this area of theory. The conclusions stated above attest to that fact. However, a final conclusion cannot be drawn regarding the relationship between the different types of communication stimuli and their interactions. In order to arrive at such a conclusion this writer conducted the main study underlying this report. Hypotheses to be Investigated Based upon the above writing and conclusions, the following hypotheses were formulated: 1. There is no difference between speaker images for- mulated for the same communicator employing either printed message, oral, visual, or combination communication stimuli. 2. Independently presented oral communication stimuli and visual communication stimuli produce the same speaker image as when they are presented simultaneously. The research design employed to test these hypothe- ses is presented in Chapter II. The results of the hypo- theses tests are found in.Chapter III. Chapter IV presents an analysis and evaluation of those results and suggests implications for further research in the area of speaker images. CHAPTER II RESEARCH DESIGN In brief, this research involved the presentation of twelve experimental stimuli to groups of college stu- dents. A speech pretested for attitude neutrality was filmed by a speaker unknown to the experimental subjects. The speaker used four different oral-physical delivery variations. Twelve groups each received one of these four stimuli and a thirteenth group served as a control group. The Ss in all cells had the speaker introduced to them. The control group read the Speaker's printed message and then completed a semantic differential on credibility di- mensions. SS in the remaining cells were divided into three communication media groups -— oral, visual and com- bination stimuli. These SS received the particular stimu- lus, read the message and then completed the differential. Summed means for the three credibility dimensions, i.e., dynamism, character and authoritativeness, were ob- tained for each cell. An analysis of variance between the media conditions was computed and individual cell compari- sons were made when the analysis of variance resulted in a Significant F-ratio. Also, an equation constructed by this 22 23 writer was tested by means of Efratios to determine whether or not oral and visual communication media stimuli addi- tively produce terminal credibility. This equation is dis- cussed further in the "Data Collection and Analysis" por- tion of this chapter. From the results of all statistical analysis of this study certain conclusions and inferences were drawn. Hypotheses Investigated The following hypotheses were investigated in the experimental research: 1. There is no difference between speaker images for- mulated for the same communicator employing either printed message, oral, visual, or combination communication stimuli. 2. Independently presented oral communication stimuli and visual communication stimuli produce the same speaker image as when they are presented simultaneously. Basically, the hypotheses asked two questions. Do audience members react differently to a Speaker when they receive his message through different communication media? Secondly, are oral and visual communication stimuli addi- tive agents in the production of terminal credibility? Operational Definitions Prior to presenting further details of the research design the following operational definitions are offered. Speaker image--Individually summed semantic differ- 24 ential scores for dynamism, character, and authoritative- ness. Dynamism--Six bipolar semantic differential items measuring the dynamicness of a communicator (see Appendix B). Character--Six bipolar semantic differential items measuring the character of a communicator (see Appendix B). Authoritativeness--Six bipolar semantic differen- tial items measuring the authoritativeness of a communica- tor (see Appendix B). Initial credibility--Perceptions of the experimen- tal 55 based upon the credibility of a source prior to ex— posure to a communicative stimuli. To obtain a near neutral level of initial credibility, three introductions were pre- tested using the semantic differential character and author- itativeness dimensions described in preliminary study two. The selected introduction's score was 26.2. All three in- troductions were above the presumed neutral point of the measure (24)--26.2, 26.9, and 28.4. The experimental speaker introduction is contained in Appendix D. Derived credibility--The image of a communicator in the minds of the respondents which was formed by the act of communicating. Terminal credibilitye-Numeric scores obtained by summing scores of six bipolar semantic differential items for each of the credibility dimensions. 25 Printed message stimuli--A message presented as a stimulus in the form of a printed speech. Oral stimuli--A message presented as a stimulus in the form of a tape recorded speech. Visual stimuli--A message presented as a stimulus in the form of a silent motion picture film. Combination stimuli--A message presented as a stimulus in the form of a sound motion picture film. Active physical--Use of appendage gestures and body movement in public speaking. Inactive physical--Lack of appendage gestures and minimal body movement in public speaking. Fluent ora1--Smooth flowing, articulate, and force- ful delivery. Non-fluent oral--Uneven flowing, inarticulate, and hesitant delivery. Experimental Group Test SS for this study were students at Michigan State University enrolled in Public Speaking, Voice and Articulation, and Justin Morrill College's "Inquiry and Expression" classes. Figure 1 presents the testing schedule of the research. Test Instrument The test instrument was a three page booklet. Page one required the respondent to indicate his sex, year in 26 HMOflmanm o>wuomcw .Hmno pcmsam «sowumcflneoo ma oouv om\m USU Doommmfi pawn maco maoum “Houucoo mm ooum mH\m Hoa HMDfimmEQ m>fluom .Hmuo ucmsam coaumcwnfioo mm ooum ma\m Hod HMOflmhnm m>wuomcw .HmHOIucode «HMDmH> wa mane ¢H\m moa Hmowmmsm w>auom .HMHOIHSDSHM Lawnmfi> NH ooum vH\m Hoa HMDflmmnm o>wuom.Jmuo ucosHMIcoc «ammo wa ooum ¢H\m Hoa Doommos pooh maco maoum “Houucoo ha ovuma va\m Hoa HMDflmmnm o>wuooaw .Hmuo “GDDHMISOG “Homo NH ovuma va\m HoH mmmwwofi coon MHSO msoum «Houucoo ma omuoa ¢H\m Hod amoflmhsm o>wuomcfl .Hmuo ucodam «Homo ma omuoa ¢H\m Hoa mommmofi Doom maso mnoum “Houucou «a ooum va\m Hoa Hmoflmanm m>fiuom .Hmuo Damsam «Hugo ma ooum «axm Hoa Hmowmmnm m>fluom .Hmuo ucoDHMIcoc «aoflumsflnsoo ma mmum MH\m Hoa HMOflmmnm m>fluomcfl .Hmuo ucosHMIco: «coaumswneou ¢H omua ma\m Hoa HMDHmazm m>flpom .Hmuo ucmsamncoc «Hmsmfl> ma mvum ma\m Hod Hmowmhsm m>wuomcw .Hmuo uSDSHMISoc «anamfl> «a oaum ma\m Hod mumpwHom cowuwpcou 2 (mafia mumo onusou ,mDHdEHum H. II. Haoo I» museum Houuaoo can Hmucoafluomxm on pououmwcwapm ucofiumouu mo onus H HMDme 27 school and number of college speech courses taken or in progress. The text of the speech from the experimental film was on page two. Page three was an eighteen item semantic differential (McCroskey, 1966c and Berlo, 33. 31., 1961). Appendix C contains a sample test booklet. Subject Matter A speech concerning legal and blackmarket adoption of babies was constructed by this writer. To test for potential attitude bias, twenty-four undergraduate students read the message and then completed the semantic differen- tial described in preliminary study two. With a hypotheti- cal neutral point of 24, the message scored 23.8. Appendix C contains the speech employed. Stimulus Preparation A non-teaching graduate student in the Department of Speech and Theatre at Michigan State University was em- ployed as the speaker in the research. This writer and the experimental speaker spent several hours practicing the delivery of four oral-physical speaking conditions. These conditions were filmed at Mich— igan State University's Instructional Media Center under the direction of Dr. Edward McCoy. The hypotheses of this study were primarily con- cerned with the main effects of communication media, not different delivery conditions. However, to strengthen the 28 generalizations made from data analysis, four different delivery conditions were employed by the experimental speaker. This writer felt that by having varying degrees of delivery proficiency presented by the experimental Speaker in the different communication media, the results of the research would not be overly influenced by a Speci- fic value laden delivery style. Thus, four delivery condi- tions were presented as experimental sub-groups within each communication media: active physical-fluent oral, inactive physical-fluent oral, inactive physical-non-fluent oral and active physical-non-fluent oral. After the filming, four graduate assistants from the Department of Speech and Theatre each viewed a single condition and attempted to objectively describe its delivery factors. The delivery conditions and the assistants' des- criptions of them are contained in Appendix G. Stimulus Presentation During the sixth week of Spring Term, 1968, 253 Michigan State University students participated in the ex- periment (224 after random exclusion). The schedule of testing is presented in Figure l (p. 26). All of the Ss were told that they were going to participate in an exper- iment attempting to create a new speaker evaluation form. Then, the semantic differential was explained to them (Appendix A). The experimental speaker was introduced to 29 the Ss via the pre-tested introduction (Appendix D). In the oral, visual, and combination conditions the SS were told to complete the first page of the test booklet and then stop. The Ss were told what specific condition stim- ulus they were going to receive, and the stimulus was pre- sented. SS were then told to turn the page and read the printed message and then complete the semantic differen- tial. In the printed message condition SS received the same orientation to the study, had the semantic differen- tial explained, were introduced to the Speaker and then told to proceed through the entire test booklet. Data Collection and Analysis The data were coded on computer cards to facilitate scoring and statistical computations. Within each media. condition chi-square scores were obtained for "year in school" (range of 1-4) and "college speech courses taken or in progress" (range of 1-6). Means for the credibility dimensions of dynamism, character and authoritativeness (each with a range of 6-42) were obtained for each media. Oral, visual, and combination media groups were equalized in size by random exclusion of Ss. The base of equaliza- tion was the number of SS in the smallest delivery sub- group within each media. An analysis of variance for all medias was computed for the dependent variables—-dynamism, character and authoritativeness. 30 To test hypothesis 1, all differences between con- ditions were tested with the E statistic when an overall F-ratio reached significance. Tests of significance were run between the printed message media (control) and the. remaining three media conditions, and, independently be- tween the oral, visual, and combination communication medias. Hypothesis 2, that the oral and visual communica- tion conditions additively equal the combination condition did not employ the normal 5 statistic. Rather, testing im- plemented an equation developed by this writer. This writer held the following assumption: "'X' topic, plus prior knowledge of a source, plus message content, plus oral communication stimulus, plus visual communication stimulus, equals combination communication stimulus, or, terminal credibility." Employing the symbol codes for these factors, the following equation was produced: Topic + PK + M + O + V = CMB (TC) The reader will recall that in stimulus presenta- tion all Ss in oral, visual, and combination conditions read the printed message after receiving the primary stim- ulus. Thus, the effect of the message theoretically can be removed from these media conditions. This was done for hypothesis 2 by subtracting the printed message means for each credibility dimension from the corresponding credibil- ity dimensions in the other media conditions. Thus, the 31 effect of the message upon the Ss' responses to the seman- tic differential became theoretically constant. Hypothesis 2 postulated that combination communica- tion stimulus Speaker images equal oral plus visual commun- ication stimuli speaker images. Following that thesis, oral equals combination stimuli minus visual stimuli and visual media equals combination stimuli minus oral stimuli. When the constant factor is removed from each condition, the following equations are produced: 1. (CMB - M) = (V - M) + (0 - M) 2. (V 1 M) (CMB - M) - (O - M) 3. (O - M) (CMB - M) - (V - M) The difference between the means for the credibil— ity dimensions in Hypothesis 2 were tested for significance by means of the following equation: _ + _. _ _ D(O-M) D(v-M) D(CMB-M) t = MSw MSw No+Nv + NCMB 2 CHAPTER III REPORTING OF RESULTS Chapter I presented two hypotheses that were tested in this experimental study. This chapter reports the re- sults of the study under three headings: (1) Effects of stimulus conditions on source credibility. (2) The addi- tive nature of oral and visual communication stimuli. (3) Secondary measures. Effects of Media on Source Credibility Hypothesis 1: There is no difference between speaker images formulated for the same communicator employ- ing either printed message, oral, visual, or combination communication stimuli. The hypothesis was rejected in two of the three credibility dimensions. Results of the analy— sis of the dynamism dimension scores indicated that there were differences among the conditions (F = 7.97, p.< .0005); results of the analysis of the authoritativeness dimension scores indicated that there were differences among the con— ditions (F = 3.17, p.< .025); but the results of the anal- ysis of the character scores did not show that there were differences among the conditions (F = .453, p.> .715). 32 33 The differences between conditions were tested with the E statistic in the dynamism and authoritativeness di- mensions as follows: printed message to oral, visual, and combination medias; oral to visual and combination medias; visual to combination media. Table 1 illustrates the re- sults of these tests. In both dimensions the oral and combination stimuli resulted in means that were closer to the hypothetical neu- tral point than were the printed message and visual commun- ication stimulus means. The data seem to indicate that a communicator may be perceived as being more dynamic and authoritative when the stimulus is only a printed message or a visually perceived speaker. This assumption appears to be substantiated most firmly in the dynamism cross con- dition comparisons. Of the six tests of comparison, only the test between the printed message condition and visual condition did not reach the required level of significance. The authoritativeness dimension of source credibil- ity produced the same result with regard to the test be- tween the printed message condition to the visual condition test. However, the reader Should observe that the oral to combination and visual to combination tests produced simi- lar results. Although it is not possible to statistically com- pare separate factors that are a product of reliable factor analysis (Berlo, 33. 31., 1961 and McCroskey, 1966c), ob- 34 TABLE 1 Dynamism and Authoritativeness Semantic Differential Cell Comparisons and Results A; Dimension Means Group N —Dynam1sm Authorita- tiveness Printed Message 72 19.27 15.94 Oral Media 48 21.39 19.18 Visual Media 48 19.16 16.50 Combination Media 56 24.42 18.10 A Dynamism 5' 2. Significance. Printed to Oral 2.12 1.726 .05 Printed to Visual .11 .089 .10 Printed to Combination 5.15 4.386 .0005 Oral to Visual 2.23 1.657 .05 Oral to Combination 3.03 2.337 .01 Visual to Combination 5.26 4.058 .0005 Authoritativeness U 2 Significance Printed to Oral 3.24 2.002 .025 Printed to Visual .56 .346 .10 Printed to Combination 2.16 1.110 .05 Oral to Visual 2.68 2.106 .025 Oral to Combination 1.08 .880 .10 Visual to Combination 1.60 1.300 .10 servation of the differences teresting. between the media means is in- The data seem to indicate, speculatively, that the dynamism dimension is more sensitive to stimulus vari- 35 ation than is authoritativeness. Differences on the former dimension failed to achieve significance in only one com-- parison while half of the differences on the latter dimen— sion failed to do so. And yet, in all corollary instances, the means of the dynamism conditions are closer to the pre— sumed hypothetical neutral point than are those of the authoritativeness dimension. The Additive Nature of Oral and Visual Communication Stimuli Hypothesis 2: Independently presented oral commun- ication stimuli and visual communication stimuli produce the same speaker image as when they are presented simul- taneously. The hypothesis was rejected on two of the three credibility dimensions. The test of the dynamism scores indicated (p.< .02) that oral and visual communication stimuli were not additive and the test of the authorita- tiveness scores indicated (p.< .05) that oral and visual communication stimuli are not additive. The test of the character scores failed to reject the hypothesis of addi- tivity, though the observed difference approached signifi- cance (p.‘<.20). Table 2 presents the main results of tests on hypothesis 2. The discussion of hypothesis 1 pointed out that the printed message and visual communication media stimuli means were consistently further from the hypothetical 36 TABLE 2 Dynamism, Character, and Authoritativeness Semantic Differ- ential Mean Difference Scores and Results Statistic Dimension 5' Error 2 Signifi- (OfAVx)-CMBx*term cance. Dynamism 3.14 1.30 2.415 .02 Character 1.32 1.01 1.307 .10 Authoritativeness 2.64 1.23 2.147 .05 neutral point than were the oral and combination stimuli means. Recalling the equation: CMB-M=(O-M)+(V—M), indi- vidual media results for each dimension prove interesting. Table 3 illustrates the stimuli mean differences for each dimension after the constant factor of printed message has been removed. The data in Table 3 clearly indicate that for the dynamism and authoritativeness dimensions of source credi- bility, oral and visual stimuli are interactive, not addi— tive agents. Because there was not a significant F-ratio produced by the analysis of variance for the character di- mension, we may not conclude that the stimuli are either additive or interactive in this dimension. No significant relationship between oral and visual communication media stimuli as they influence the character of a communicator was observed in this study. 37 TABLE 3 Dynamism, Character, and Authoritativeness.Semantic Differ- ential Means Adjusted to Constant Message Factor ___ Dimensions Condition DynamISm Char— Authorita- acter tiveness Printed Message 19.27 18.12 15.94 Oral Media minus Message - 2.12 .29 3.24 Visual Media minus Message .11 1.10 .56 Combination Media minus Message ' 5.15 .07 2.16 Adjusted Oi—Vi- - 2.01** 1.39 4.80* *Significant at the .05 level or beyond. **Significant at the .05 level or beyond. It should be noted that on the dynamism and author- itativeness dimensions the combination stimuli consistent- ly produces a mean further from the presumed hypothetical neutral point than do the combined independent oral and visual stimuli. The addition of the communicator's inde- pendent oral stimulus to his independent visual stimulus image produced a poorer total image than did his combina- tion stimulus image alone. The visual stimulus appears to be an interactive agent tending to off-set the negative effect of the oral stimulus, but is not powerful enough to have a significant effect.’ Results on the character dimension of source credi- bility indicate the exact opposite effects for the inter- 38 action between the oral and visual stimuli. But, it must be remembered that a significant F-ratio was not achieved on this condition. Secondary Measures SS were asked to indicate their sex, year in school, and number of college speech courses taken or in progress on the test booklet. These secondary measures' were employed to ascertain whether or not the results of statistical tests may have been influenced by biased ex- perimental group composition. It was desired that the sex ratio be as close to 1:1 as possible, that the Ss be varied in their year in school, and that the number of speech courses they had taken was minimal. Table 4 reports the Chi-square values for these factors. The data obtained from Table 4 shows that the over- all sex ratio was not significantly different. The between media and year in school Chi-square scores also indicated no significant differences. In the number of college speech courses taken or in progress, Chi-square analysis showed that there was a significant difference between the number of Ss who had had only one course and those who had had two or more courses across experimental conditions. Interpretation of the above results and implica- tions for further research are presented in the succeeding chapter. 39 TABLE 4 Chi-square Values and Probabilities for Secondary Measures Condition Frequencies: Expected/Observed Variables Message Oral Visual Combin- ation Sex Male 31.5/33 20.5/16 20.5/20 24.4/29 Female 40.5/39 27.0/32 27.0/28 31.5/27 x2=3.24- Year in School Freshman» 34.0/32 22.7/26 22.7/23 26.5/25 Sophomore 13.5/12 9.0/ 6 9.0/14 10.5/10 Junior 18.6/25 12.4/13 12.4/ 8 14.5/13 Senior 5.7/ 3 3.4/ 3 3.4/ 3 44.5/ 8 x2=13.7o Speech Courses One 35.6/63 36.6/34 36.6/31 34.7/45 Two or more 16.3/ 9 10.9/14 10.9/17 12.7/11 x2=22.42* *Significant at the .01 level or beyond. CHAPTER IV CONCLUSIONS, SUBJECTIVE OBSERVATIONS, IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH AND SUMMARY In the previous chapters we reviewed the literature pertinent to a study of speaker images and reported the de- sign of an experimental study and the results of that study. It is the purpose of this chapter to: (1) present this writer's objective analysis of those results, (2) offer subjective comments pertinent to the objective re- sults of the study, (3) discuss some implications for fu- ture research in the field of speaker-image-media-research, and (4) summarize the complete project. COnclusions Analysis of Experimental Group Composition Chi-square tests were computed for the data from the secondary measures to ascertain whether or not the composition of the experimental groups may have biased the results of the study. It was hoped that there be no dif- ference in the ratio of men to women among the various ex- perimental cells in the study. Of the total number of Ss 40 41 in the study, 43.6 per cent were men and 56.4 per cent were women. Chi-square analysis indicated no more than chance' variation in the sex ratio among the various treatment con— ditions. It was hoped that the Ss' year in school would be relatively diverse and this end result was also achieved. Preliminary study two, reported in Chapter I, suggested that there is no difference between Speaker images formu- lated by trained and by naive judges. However, it was hoped that the Ss would have little experience in perfor- mance speech courses on the college level. The differences in the number of speech courses taken or in progress on the college level among the experimental conditions were sig- nificant (xzp.=<.01). However, it must be remembered that the experiment was run little more than half-way through a ten week term and thus, the majority (77%) of the subjects had not completed one college speech performance course. The reader can obtain a fairly comprehensive picture of the composition of the experimental groups by referring to Table 4 in Chapter III. The results of the Chi-square computations suggest that the study was not biased by the composition of the experimental cells. The study was apparently not influen- ced by an overloading of males or females and the ratio of Ss from each academic year was constant throughout the experimental cells within each communication media stimuli. Though there was a substantially different proportion of 42 "naive" to "trained" subjects employed in the research, the- previous experiences of this writer lead him to-believe that this factor had little, if any, effect on the results of the present study. Analysis of Media Effects Results of tftests between the different communica- tion stimuli conditions were applicable for only the dyna- miSm and authoritativeness dimensions of source credibility. Respectively, these dimensions had F-ratios of 7.97, p.‘<.0005 and 3.17, p.<:.025. The character dimension of source credibility had an F-ratio of .453, p.> .715. In- dividually, and comparatively, the tested dimensions pro- duced interesting results. Qynamism.--The only cross—communication stimuli test which did not produce an acceptable level of signifi- cance was the printed message to visual test. The oral communication stimulus was significantly different from printed message (p.‘<.05), visual (p.<:.05) and combination (p.‘<.01). The visual stimulus condition was significantly different from combination (p.< .0005) and combination was significantly different from printed message (p. <.0005). With five of the six tests producing significant differ- ences in the dynamism dimension of source credibility, it is readily apparent that communicators must realize that different communication stimuli can differentially affect one aspeCt of their terminal credibility. 43 Authoritativeness.--The cross-communication media stimuli tests in this dimension of source credibility did not produce acceptable levels of Significant differences in the printed message to visual, oral to combination, and visual to combination stimuli conditions. Tests which did indicate significant differences were those between oral to printed message (p. < .025), oral to visual (p.< .025), and printed message to combination (p. <.05). Though only half of the tests in the authoritativeness dimension of source credibility produced significant Erratios, this writer feels that the admonition in the discussion of the dynamism di- mension is again appropriate. Communicators must realize that different communication stimuli can differentially affect aspects of terminal credibility. Conclusions.--Although statistical tests between source credibility dimensions are of dubious validity, some observations are appropriate. More of the dynamism dimen- sion cross-communication media stimuli tests achieved levels of significance than did those on the authoritaé tiveness dimension. These results suggest that on the whole, all media stimuli affect dynamism more than they do authoritativeness in terms of a communicator's source cre- dibility. 44 Analysis of Oral-Visual Communication~Media Stimuli Additivity Employing the basic formula CMB-M = (O-M)+(V-M) discussed in Chapter II, the following conclusions were drawn: (1) Oral and visual communication media stimuli are not additive in producing terminal credibility in the dyna- mism and authoritativeness dimensions of source credibili- ty. (2) Oral and visual communication media stimuli, if relevant at all, may be additive in producing terminal credibility in the character dimension of source credibil- ity. Potential Confounding Vari- abIeS to be Considered Several factors should be considered when viewing the results and conclusions of this experimental study. The introduction used to introduce the experimen- tal Speaker to the Ss may have biased the results of the study. This is not likely, however, as the introduction was pretested for attitude neutrality and scored 26.2 with a hypothetical neutral point of 24. Also, if we can assume that Ss' knowledge about a speaker is reflected by Ss on a semantic differential in the character dimension of source credibility, then the introduction did not bias the results as the analysis of variance was not significant in that di- mension (p.< .715). 45 Delivery types may have been overloaded in one style for one.or all of the media and this may have biased the results of the study. This factor should have been in- operative as random exclusion of Ss used the smallest num— ber of Ss within each delivery type as its base. Thus, for the printed message condition there was no random exclusion as all SS received the same stimulus. Each delivery style cell was equal in size to all other experimental cells within its particular treatment condition. The number of times anvS received the experimental speaker's message may have biased the results of the study. Recall that the effect of the message was theoretically re- moved from the oral, visual, and combination stimuli con— ditions once. However, even after this constant factor had been removed, a message residual may have been retained. For instance, in the oral stimulus condition the Ss heard and then read the experimental message, and_yet, the con- stant factor was removed only once. The same situation would apply to the combination stimulus condition. Only in the visual stimulus condition was constant factor removal equal to number of message inputs. The results of this study are not purported to be in any sense definitive be— cause of this potential confounding variable. Even though the experimental message employed in this research was pretested for neutrality, the content may have made the experimental Speaker appear to be very author- 46 itative. This effect was not considered in secondary measures testing and it may have biased the results of the study. Subjective Observations As Marshall MacLuen has said, "The media is the message." That statement and the results of this study would seem to have some far-reaching implications for the politician, businessman, educator, and layman alike. The aforementioned image thesis which Senator Hatfield said needs more investigating, needs just that. Vance Packard in The Hidden Persuaders tells us that businessmen-are con- cerned about their communicator images as advertisers. The advent of educational television has given educators a man- date to consider the images they may foster. Laymen must realize that what they read, see, and hear creates in their minds particular communicator images. With more knowledge of the "whys" and "hows" of media and delivery interaction, it is likely that communicators will be able to more.pro- fitably alter their images than they have previously thought possible and/or plausable. The conclusions of this study, in the writer's opinion, seem to have significant ramifications for com- municators. One seldom thinks of the different effects his communication may have on receivers when that message is presented via different communication media. Results of 47 this study lead this writer to believe that caution, or at least consideration, should be exercised by a communicator intending to utilize different communication media. The results of our tests of hypothesis 2 suggest that if options were available, a communicator might find it more advantageous only to be seen then to have his mes- sage read. From this point of view, politicians should distribute picture-leaflets rather than printed speeches. Following the results relating to hypothesis 2 further, it would seem that communicators might discover a more favor- able image if they had their messages read, rather than heard; were seen, rather than heard when communicating; and heard, rather than having their messages read and then ber ing simultaneously seen and heard delivering the same mes- sage. The significant results relating to hypothesis 1, that terminal source credibility is affected differentially by different communication media stimuli, might be attrib- uted to the effect of the combining of several different delivery stimuli within each media. This possibility does exist and this writer would speculate that data obtained from cross-communication media tests which had employed distinctly different delivery styles would produce results showing significant interactions between media types and delivery styles. For instance, if a communicator's only stimulus was oral, and he possessed "good" oral and "poor" 48 physical delivery traits, this writer believes that his auditors might be able to subconsciously perceive the phys- ical aspects. Conversely, if the communicator were only seen, and had "good" physical and "poor" oral delivery traits, the oral factors might somehow influence receivers' reactions toward him. In other words, visual communication media stimuli-may carry with it latent cues which have been generated by oral delivery. This writer feels that the same would hold true for the oral stimuli--in some manner it carries with it cues related to physical activity. Because of the differences between media and the apparent interaction of oral and visual communication media stimuli, this writer believes that delivery will have an interactive effect on types of communication media. He would also speculate that specific different gestures and oral inflections have effects quite different than styles which have few gestures and limited oral inflection. Oral and visual communication stimuli seem to be interactive rather than additive in producing terminal credibility. This writer would question why the oral media stimulus becomes a negative factor when combined with a visual media stimulus. Also, why is the off-setting inter- active effect of the visual media stimulus so minimal? Even though the assumption of this writer, that behavioral communication research is erring by not using the visual stimuli more in its investigations, has apparently been 49 rejected by this study, he still feels that the visual media stimulus is more important than this study has shown. There are too many evident physical cues to poor oral de- livery for them to be sublimated. Physiological reactions to oral dysfluencies are quite common--casting the eyes up- ward or snapping one's fingers to recall the "right" word, an abrupt checking of notes when a word has been mispro- nounced, biting the lip or flipping the hands during a thought pause--the list seems endless. Surely these are cues which make an impression in the minds of receivers, even if they have only viewed a communicator. Subjective contemplations on the results of this study are intriguing. This writer suggests that answers to some of the media and delivery variables discussed above should be isolated and identified. This writer believes that it would be possible to transgress beyond subjectivity and benefit communicators by discovering what types of fac- tors precipitate the oral-visual communication stimuli in- teraction. Implications for Future Research The experimental study, results, and conclusions reported above provide some relevant data for the study of the effects of different communication stimuli upon the credibility of a communicator. Two questions were tenti- tively answered with the following statements: (1) There so are differences in perceived communicator credibility at- tributable to the media through which the stimulus is pre- sented. (2) In at least two source credibility dimensions, dynamism and authoritativeness, oral and visual communica- tion stimuli are not additive in producing terminal credi- bility. There is a need for studies of cumulative replica- tion. For instance, the study of media effects on source credibility would profit by knowing if the different media produce constant effects and if media and delivery produce constant effects. Several profitable studies could be designed. Such research might: 1. Study the interaction between different media stim- uli and delivery styles. 2. Study the interaction between different media stim- uli and specific delivery features, such as gestures and oral inflections. 3. Study the interaction between different media stim- uli, delivery styles and amount of message removal. 4. Study the interaction between different media stim- uli, delivery styles and message removal when two speakers are used as experimental stimuli. The possibilities might include tests of matched and cross-matched experimental speaker cells. The four research possibilities stated above would 51 investigate some of the most basic aspects of communication media and delivery interaction. Another realm could be opened with the inclusion-of other variables, such as one- side, two-side arguments in the messages, credibility man- ipulation, climactic order and similar types of variables. Summary Richard Nixon's defeat in the 1960 Presidential election is commonly attributed to the poor image he pre- sented in the televised debates with John Kennedy. Whe- ther or not one accepts this notion, he must realize that; many political leaders "believe that personality and image- making are the most powerful forces in swaying the elector- ate" (Kraus, 1962, p. 162). Behavioral research in persuasive communication accepts the position that either the image or the message will ultimately emerge as the dominant agent of value acti- vation for the listener. Whichever factor dominates de- termines the basic nature of the persuasive act. The re- sults of the study of kinesics suggest that the nonverbal communication of a source may reveal subconscious motiva- tions. This writer's review of the literature pertinent to the study of speaker images led him to believe that repor- ted research in the field did not consider two major areas: (1) Behavioral research employs different communication 52 medias to present stimuli for testing hypotheses regarding various communication variables without knowing the intra- cacies of the effects of those medias. (2) Communication research commonly employs only the oral communication media stimulus to test hypotheses regarding content and delivery. This writer felt that in doing so, research was neglecting the important factor of the communicator's visual communi- cation. This writer conducted preliminary experimental re- search and the major study reported in this writing to examine the two areas stated above. Two hypotheses were tested in the major experimental study: (1) There is no difference between speaker images formulated for the same communicator employing either printed message, oral, vis- ual, or combination communication stimuli. (2) Independent- ly presented oral communication stimuli and visual commun- ication stimuli produce the same speaker image as when they are presented simultaneously. Four delivery styles were presented to Ss through four communication media--printed message, oral, visual, and combination of oral and visual. Semantic differential measures on three source credibility dimensions, dynamism, character, and authoritativeness, were obtained from exper- imental Ss after they were introduced to the experimental» Speaker and had received his message. Chi-square scores were obtained from the Ss for certain demographic factors. 53 Dependent variable data was subjected to analysis of vari- ance. Tftests for between media effects were computed where significant F-ratios were obtained. To test the ad- ditive nature of oral and visual communication stimuli, t: tests were also computed. Results of these analyses suggested the following generalizations: (1) The composition of the experimental groups did not appear to bias the results of the study. (2) Favorability of speaker credibility on the authorita- tiveness and dynamism dimensions varies with the media through which a receiver receives a communication stimulus. (3) In two of the source credibility dimensions, dynamism and authoritativeness, oral and visual communication media stimuli are interactive, not additive, agents in the pro- duction of terminal credibility. (4) On the character di- mension of source credibility, oral and visual communica- tion media stimuli, if applicable at all, appear to be nei- ther additive or interactive in the production of terminal credibility. The experimental study was designed to discover any differences between Speaker images attributable to varying the media of stimulation and the additive or inter- active nature of oral and visual communication stimuli. At present there is an awareness of the different oral, visual, and nonverbal communicator images. Previous research had investigated other types of images, from po- 54 litical profiles to psychological personality traits. Re- search on ethos and studies in the field of psychiatry suggest the importance of the verbal and nonverbal images. However, no research had attempted to determine the rela- tive differences between these images in their relationships to terminal credibility. It would appear to this writer that the information gathered by this study was-needed for communication research to promulgate more effective theory. This study was designed to answer some questions in this regard. It does not purport to be all-inclusive. The writer recognizes that it is but one of the steps nec- essary to obtain information to answer the questions which will arise in connection with media-speaker image research. Only cumulative studies will provide firm answers to the basic problems approached in this study. By design the study was limited to testing college students under favorable conditions. Thus, the results may not be widely generalized until similar studies employing different populations support the findings of this re- search. It is the hope of this writer that more refined research will be conducted into the variables related to nonverbal communication and source credibility. REFERENCES Allport, G. W. and Cantrill, H. Judging personality from voice. Journal of Social Psychology, 1934, 5, 37-55. Benham, T. Polling for a presidential candidate. Public Qpinion_guarter1y, 1965, 29, 185-199. Berelson, B. and Janowitz, M. Reader in Public Opinion and Communication, New York: The Free Press, 1966. Berlo, D. K., Lemert, J. B., and Mertz, R. J. Dimensions for evaluating the acceptability of message sources. Unpublished paper based on a paper presented at the igzich Association of America.Convention, New York, Birdwhistle, R. L. Background to Kinesics. Etc., 1955, 13’ 10-180 Brilhart, B. L. Speaker message perception and attitude change as a function of field independence. S eech Monographs, 1967, 34, 222-223. Doctoral dissertation abstract, Pennsylvania State University, 1966. Davitz, Joel R. and Lois Jean. The communication of feel- ings by content—free speech. Journal of Communication, 1959, 9, 6-13. Eckman, P. Body position, facial expression and verbal be- havior during interviews. Journal of Abnormal Psych- ology, 1964, 68, 295-301. Ehrensberger, R. An experimental study of the relative effectiveness of certain forms of emphasis in public speaking. Speech Monographs, 1960, 27, 94-111. Gardiner, J. C. An experimental study of the use of selec- ted forms of evidence in effecting attitude change. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Nebraska, 1966. Greenburg, B. S. and Miller, G. R. The effects of low credible sources on message acceptance. Speech Mono- graphs, 1966, 33, 127-136. 55 56 Greenhill, L. P. Application of sound motion pictures to research in clinical psychology. Audio-Visual Commun- ication Review, 1955, 5, 529-539. Giedt, F. H. Cues associated with accurate and inaccurate interview impressions. Psychiatry, 1958, 21, 405-409. . Visual, content and auditory cues in interview- ing. Journal of ConsultingAPsychology, 1955, 19, 407- 416. Haiman, F. S. An experimental study of the effects of ethos in public Speaking. Speech Monographs, 1949, 16, 191-202. Harms, L. S. Social judgments of status cues in language. Speech Monographs, 1960, 27, BL -Ph.D. dissertation ab- stract, Ohio State University, 1959. Hatfield, M. 0. Personal interview by Ray E. Weisenborn, November 17, 1967. Hildreth, R. A. An experimental study of audiences' abil- ity to distinguish between Sincere and insincere speeches. Speech Monographs, 1954, 21, 146. Doctoral dissertation abstract, UnIVersity of Southern Califor- nia, 1953. Jersild, A. Modes of emphasis in public speaking. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1928, 12, 611-620. King, T. R. An experimental study of the effects of ethos upon the immediate and delayed recall of information. Central States Speech Journal, 1966, 17, 22-28. Kraus, S. The Great Debates. Bloomington: Indiana Uni- versity Press, 1962. Krestinger, E. A. An experimental study of gross bodily movement as an index to audience interest. Speech Monographs, 1952, 19, 244-248. Levonian, E. The use of film in opinion measurement. Audio-Visual Communication Review, 1962, 10, 250-254. McCroskey, J. C. An Introduction to Rhetorical Communica- tion. New Jersey: Prentice Hall, 1968a. , Pritchard, V. O. and Arnold, W. E., Attitude in- tensity and the semantic differential. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1967-1968b, 4, 242-245. 57 , and Dunham, R. E. Ethos: a confounding factor in communication research. Speech Monographs, 1966a, 33, 456-463. . Experimental studies in the effects of ethos and eVIdence in persuasive communication. Unpublished Doc- toral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, 1966b, (Abstract: Speech Monographs, 1967, 34, 254). . Studies of the effects of evidence in persuasive communication. Speech Communication Research Labora- tory, Michigan State University, 1967. . Scales for the measurement of ethos. Speech Monographs, 1966c, 33, 65-72. McGrath, J. and M. The effects of partisanship on the per- ceptions of political figures. Public Opinion Quarter-. 1.x, 1962' 26' 236-2480 Michael, W. and Crawford, C. C. An experiment in judging intelligence by the voice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1925, 18, 107-114. Miller, G. R. and Hewgill, M. A. The effect of variations in non-fluency on audience ratings of source credibil- ity. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 1964, 50, 36-44. Nadeau, R. Delivery in ancient times. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 1964, 50, 50-63. Neubauer, J. The camera and JFK. Popular Photography, 1967, 61, 88-102. Reusch, Jurgen and Kees. Nonverbal Communication. Berke- ley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1954. Rosenthal, P. I. Ethos in the presidential campaign of 1960: a study of the basic persuasive process of the Kennedy-Nixon televised debates. Speech Monogra hs, 1964, 31, 257. Doctoral dissertation abstract, Uni- versity of California, 1963. . The concept of ethos and the structure of per- suasion. Speech Monographs, 1966, 33, 114-126. Sapir, E. Speech as a personality trait. American Journal of Sociology, 1927, 32, 892-905. 58 Sherman, D. The merits of backward playing of connected speech in the scaling of voice quality disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1954, 19, 312-321. Sigel, R. The effects of partisanship on the perceptions of political figures. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1964, 28, 483-496. Thomkins, S. and Izard, C. E. (eds.). Affect, Cognition and Personality. New York: Springer, 1965. Weaver, C. H. and G. L. Information theory and the measure- ment of meaning. Speech Monographs, 1965, 32, 244-248. Weisenborn, R. E. An exploratory study of speaker images. Unpublished research paper, Department of Sociology, Michigan State University, December, 1967. White, F. C. The political image makers. Public Broad- casting Laboratory. Panel discussion televiSed’On November 19, 1967. Winthrop, H. Effects of personal qualities on one-way communication. Psychological Reports, 1956, 324-334. Williams, F. and Sundene, B. Dimensions of recognition: visual vs. vocal expression of emotion. Audio-Visual Communication Review, 1965, 13, 44-52. APPENDIX A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL EXPLANATION The semantic differential is an attitude measuring instrument. It consists of a concept, such as "Michigan State University," and below the concept are two columns of adjectives separated by seven spaces or blanks. The adjec- tives are bi-polan’in other words, they have opposite mean- ings, such as, "good" and "bad". You are to indicate how you feel about the concept for each pair of adjectives. The middle, or fourth space is neutral, and you should place an."x" there is you feel neither adjective applies to the concept or if they both apply equally. When you- feel that an adjective does apply to the concept, indicate your feeling by placing an "x" toward or away from the ad- jective on the scale. For instance, with the concept "Michigan State University" and the bi-polars "large- small" you would probably place your "x" in one of the three spaces between the word "large" and the scale's neutral point. 59 Aggressive Emphatic Forceful Bold Active Energetic Unselfish Nice Friendly Honest Pleasant Virtuous APPENDIX B SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL IN CREDIBILITY DIMENSIONS Dynamism 60 Meek Hesitant Forceless Timid Passive Tired Selfish Awful Unfriendly Dishonest Unpleasant Sinful 61 Authoritativeness Informed :_:_:_:_:__:_:_: Uninformed Qualified :___;___5___}___}___:___:___: Unqualified Reliable :__:_:__:__:__:__:___: Unreliable Valuable -___;___;___fi___- : : : Worthless Intelligent : : : : : : : : Unintelligent Expert : : : : : : : : Inexpert APPENDIX C RESEARCH TEST BOOKLET Male Female Indicate academic standing: (circle) Frosh. Soph. Junior Senior Indicate courses taken or in progress: Basic Speech Group Discussion Communication course(s) Oral Interpretation Argumentation Persuasion 62 63 Speech by Mr. Robinson A few years ago childless couples hopefully turned to adoption agencies only to be turned away because of the strict regulations which resulted from the shortage of a- doptable children. Many of these couples refused to take a polite "no" for an answer and searched elsewhere for a child. The result has been a boom in black market babies. The pressure of black markets has forced the adoption agen- cies to retreat. They have been widely criticized -- for piling on red tape, snooping, prying, and for "playing God" in selecting those who will receive babies. The demand for young infants has put pressure on adoption agencies and made it necessary for them to alter their restrictions. They now realize that couples who will adopt children independently are willing to take a number of risks in order to obtain children. These couples not only take risks by breaking the law, but they also chance the possibility of thwarting a child's personality. Cou- ples who want intelligent and responsive chidren must wait until the child is old enough for psychological tests to be effective. Granted, an agency is responsible for trying to find a child for prospective adOptive parents. But, their main focus is on finding a family for a child rather than a child for a family. It must be remembered that those who would sell an infant on the black market take 64 neither the parent-to-be or child into consideration. Their main aim is the turning of a dollar. Couples who wish to adopt a child must protect themselves. It is the job of the agencies to provide homeless children with good homes and reliable parents. Social workers must be in constant touch with prospective parents. The more a person can reveal about the way he functions in his work with his family and friends, the better the social worker can predict how he will function as a parent. And, adOptive parents have a great responsibility. A good adop- tive parent is someone who can love and enjoy children, is reasonably happily married, can face life realistically, is mature, stable, and yet flexible enough to roll with the punches. A good adoptive parent.must have an extra dimen- sion. He must be able to accept that his child was born to someone else, and tolerate the often painful but inalter- able facts of the child's heredity and background. The latter points can be adjusted much more readily by adoption agencies. They have the facilities to match the "best" child with the "best" parents. Until recently there has always been a demand for more babies. But the tables have turned, and now for the first time in thirty years there are more children to be adopted than there are people to adopt them. Many factors contribute to this fact; the rate of illegitimacy has been on the rise, pe0ple seem to be marrying at a younger age 65 and having "unwanted" children, medical science has made. infertility a thing of the past. To correct the "parent gap" adoption agencies will have to remodel their adoptive laws. If they do not, black market sales of babies are- sure to skyrocket. They must remove religious barriers, expand agency facilities, place children at an earlier age, place more than one child in the same family, concern themselves less with matching physical characteristics and insisting on a substantial bank account or ownership of a home as requirements for adoptive parents. Adoption is a lifetime decision. A valid adoption decree creates legal relationships which are the same as if the child had been born to his new parents. Only legal agencies can safeguard child and family in this regard. Aggressive Hesitant Forceful Timid Active Tired Unselfish Awful Friendly Dishonest Pleasant Sinful Informed Unqualified Reliable Worthless Intelligent Inexpert 66 ROBINSON AS A PUBLIC SPEAKER MR. Meek Emphatic Forceless Bold Passive Energetic- Selfish Nice Unfriendly Honest Unpleasant Virtuous Uninformed Qualified Unreliable Valuable Unintelligent Expert APPENDIX D INTRODUCTION OF EXPERIMENTAL SPEAKER Mr. Michael Robinson is a native of Bloomington, Indiana. Upon graduating from high school he attended Bowling Green University for one year. During the next four years he was employed by a youth center in Wisconsin. Mr. Robinson was then an activities director for the YMCA in Racine, Wisconsin. He held the job at the YMCA for six years and for the next three years was on a work-study program for non-degree holding personnel in Wisconsin's Welfare Depart- ment. Mr. Robinson was transferred to the Social Work main office for five years and for the past two years has been a social worker assistant. 67 APPENDIX E ORAL-PHYSICAL DELIVERY STIMULI DESCRIPTIONS Fluent-Oral-Active Physical -- This Speaker's vocal quality exhibits rich resonance, with lengthy pauses be- tween sentences. The volume is moderate, and tends to drop low at sentence endings. The articulation is clear. The speaker uses many "half gestures" with either hand (some-- times both)--jabbing, pointing, and chopping. He divideS‘ his time between looking at his notes and looking at his audience. He rocks from side to side, and his facial ex- pression is always serious. Fluent Oral-Inactive Physical -- The speaker uses very little variety in all delivery qualities. Vocal em- phasis patterns, gestures and bodily movements are repeti- tive and constant throughout the speech. The gestures used are small expressive hand movements with the arms remaining in a bent position, seldom being removed from the lecturn. Bodily expression consists of small nervous head and shoul- der movements. The speaker appears to be very sincere. His facial expression and vocal tone contribute to this image. Throughout the Speech the speaker relies heavily on his notes and often looks down--eye contact is minimal. 68 69 Non-fluent Oral-Inactive Physical -- Throughout the speech the speaker was largely confined to notes. At points where there was an attempt to get away from them and engage in eye contact, loss of train of thought often re- sulted. Little in the way of gestures; one major gesture with the right hand. The left hand used to graSp the lec- turn throughout the speech. Many non-fluencies noted: they seemed to consist of (1) loss of place in notes, thus mix- up of wording, or (2) search for the appropriate word, lending some indication that he was attempting to be extem- poraneous. Speaking voice largely uninteresting. Tone tended toward a single pitch level with the inflection pat- tern varied little. Toward the end of the speech the Speak- er seemed to be more relaxed, have more inflection and an extemporaneous tone. Non-fluent Oral-Active Physical -- The speaker used frequent hand gestures accompanied by frequent body move- ments from side to side. Eye contact with the audience was infrequent. The speaker had his eyes on his manuscript during the major portion of the speech. Several times dur- ing the speech the speaker lost his place while Speaking and became verbally non-fluent, injecting vocal pauses-- "uhm's"--until he again found his place and went on with the speech. The Speaker's voice followed a pattern of be- ing loud, then soft. The speaker's verbal delivery also followed a very broken up pattern of intonation by pausing 70 in the middle of sentences as well as between sentences of a Single thought pattern. The speaker mispronounced sever- al words as well as not vocalizing the endings of several other words. APPENDIX F ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE RESULTS J r Dynamism Category Mean Frequency Sum of Standard Squares Deviation Printed Message 19.27 72 29028.0 5.654 Oral Media 21.39 48 25139.0 8.206 Visual Media 19.16 48 19652.0 6.553 Combination Media 24.42 56 35530.0 6.196 (over-all) 20.99 224 109349.0 6.896 Source of Variance Mean F Sta- Approx. Significancev Square tistic Probab. of F Statistic Between Categories 346.896 7.977 .0005 Within Categories 43.483 Character Category Mean Frequency Sum of Standard , Squares Deviation Printed Message 18.12 72 25063.0 4.456 Oral Media 19.22 48 19567.0 6.220 Visual Media 18.41 48 17286.0 4.625 Combination Media 18.48 56 20715.0 5.369 (over-all) 18.51 224 82631.0 5.124 Source of Variance Mean F Sta- Approx. Significance Square tistic Probab. of F Statistic Between Categories 11.985 0.453 .715 Within Categories 26.454 71 72 Authoritativeness Category Mean Frequency Sum of Standard Squares Deviation Printed Message 15.94 72 20366.0 5.388 Oral Media 19.18 48 20493.0 7.747 Visual Media 16.50 48 14414.0 5.351 Combination Media 18.10 56 20692.0 6.510 (over-all) 17.29 224 75965.0 6.328 Source of Variance Mean F Sta- Approx. Significance Square tistic Probab. of F Statistic Between Categories 123.504 3.174 .025 Within Categories 38.911 VITA Ray Edward Weisenborn was born on July 10, 1941 in Portland, Oregon. After being graduated from Battle Ground High School, Battle Ground, Washington in June, 1959, he attended Pacific Lutheran University, Tacoma, Washington for one year. The Associate of Arts degree was granted to him by Clark College, Vancouver, Washington in June, 1961. During two years of study at Portland State College, Portland, Oregon, he was active in student activities and the Speech and Drama Departments. The Bachelor of Science degree was awarded him in June, 1963. During the following year he taught English, speech, and drama at Sutherlin High School, Sutherlin, Oregon. Prior to beginning graduate studies at Eastern New Mexico University, Portales, New Mexico, he was a resident actor in that institution's Corrall Playhouse. The Master of Arts degree was awarded to him in Speech and Drama by that institution in June, 1965. During the academic year 1965-66 he was an Instructor in the Speech and Drama De- partment at Clark College. Mr. Weisenborn began doctoral studies at Michigan State University in September, 1966. He was a graduate teaching assistant during the 1966-67 academic year and 73 74 during 1967-68 was the Administrative Assistant for the Speech 101 program, under the direction of Dr. David C. Ralph. Mr. Weisenborn was also an Instructor in Justin Morrill College during the 1967-68 academic year. Mr. Weisenborn is a member of the dramatic honor- aries Junior Collegiate Players and Alpha Psi Omega and the speech honorary Pi Kappa Delta. He is an active member of the Speech Association of America, National Education Asso- ciation, Association of Higher Education, American Associa- tion of University Professors and the Benevolent and Pro- tective Order of Elks. Mr. Weisenborn is married to the former Donna Marie Graves of Vancouver, Washington.