TRAINING OF RESIDENT ASRIRTIINFS FIND PEER GROUP RIF-RIDERS IN THE COMMUNICATION INTERACTIONIII. PROCESS PRIIID OF FIIIPRTIIID UNDERSTANDING OF STUDENT FEELING RND STUDENT DEPTH DF SELF EXPLORATION ’ ‘ 7 Them for the Degree Of, PTI D ' MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ’ ‘ ' ' ‘ 1971 LIBRARY Michigan Stan: llnivcnfity This is to certify that the thesis entitled Training of Resident Assistants and Peer Group Members in the Communication Interactional Process Skills of Empathic Understanding of Student Feeling and Student Depth of Self-Exploration presented by Kathleen Rosina Scharf has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. degree in magi—Counseling Date August 13, 1971 0-7639 ”'5‘ v‘.. 8.5‘¢.‘-i "It; Min- 'r! U CSS I ~ ‘ V‘ urflergr ‘11.“ | I ‘h I I€.pers‘ T l A“, “us. Two tr EXP‘Frie ABSTRACT TRAINING OF RESIDENT ASSISTANTS AND PEER GROUP MEMBERS IN THE COMMUNICATION INTERACTIONAL PROCESS SKILLS OF EMPATHIC UNDERSTANDING OF STUDENT FEELING AND STUDENT DEPTH OF SELF-EXPLORATION BY Kathleen Rosine Scharf The purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness of two different approaches to the training of undergraduate paraprofessional mental health workers or "helpers." This investigation attempted to determine the kinds of trainee behavior evidenced following an intensive 40-hour training program conducted during five consecutive days. Two training programs were used--a modified IPR experience and a modified communications skills train- ing program. The outcomes of both treatments were com- pared to each other, to a comparison group of resident assistants trained extensively in a 38—hour IPR program which was conducted over a six-month period of time, and to a comparison group of professional counselors. Both the experimental training procedures—-the modified IPR experience and the modified communication skills program--were designed to increase the "interpersonal communicatzo: 1‘ ““"vv gcals unatii; ‘ 1 ...' .. fine fCliOwiT.: tlve to the : Rise trainees I.- . “‘ " , much would : ghe trainees :ELFQI‘St'Jieg‘ hall. 'uyer‘h wall and help I er S ’10:}: Kathleen Rosine Scharf communication skills" of student helpers. Four general goals underlying both of the training programs included the following: to make trainees more aware of and sensi- tive to the feelings and concerns of their peers; to pro- vide trainees with listening and communication skills which would facilitate understanding between them and the students desiring help; to offer student helpers effective ways of responding to and behaving with students; and to give trainees an increased awareness of the impact of the helper-student interaction in the college and residence hall. Therefore the "interpersonal communication skills" of interest in this study included both helper empathic understanding and the helper's ability to use affective, understanding, specific, and eXploratory responses to student concerns. Helper empathic understanding was de- fined as the helper's sensitivity to current feelings, as evidenced by his verbal facility to communicate this understanding to a student in a language attuned to the peer's current feelings (Carkhuff, 1969b). Helper affec- tive responses made reference to the emotions, feelings, or fears of a client. Understanding responses communi- cated to the student that the helper understood or was trying to understand what the student was eXpressing, both verbally and non-verbally. Specific responses were the helper's honest statements of the core concerns being Dresenteu. C; C3163 meme; cent to EV" ‘ :1!“ ‘ ' v"“"‘ w- .U“O*‘..H “.3:- J ‘4. ‘ Kathleen Rosine Scharf presented, either explicitly or implicitly, verbally or non-verbally, by the student. Exploratory responses indi- cated whether the helper encouraged or permitted the stu— dent to explore his concerns. This study compared the relative effectiveness of the Carkhuff and the Kagan training models when used with undergraduate paraprofessional "helpers." In this in- vestigation, both training models were modified in the following manner. While Carkhuff usually included the five dimensions of E, R, G, C, and Ex in his five-step training process, this study focused on only two dimen— sions--the E and Ex scales. Therefore, the modified Carkhuff training model in this study included discrimi- nation training using two rating scales (E and Ex), empathy training, role-playing, group feedback and discussion, and client interviews. The training program was conducted as an intensive 40—hour experience over a period of five days. Kagan's IPR process usually included a lecture on facilitative conditions, stimulated confrontation recall, Client or counselor recall, and mutual recall. This study Imodified the IPR process in the following ways. The CVRS Was not used in the lecture on facilitative conditions. The E and Ex scales were used instead. In this way, the C3VRS could be used as an unbiased criterion measure across aldl groups. Stimulation confrontation recall (filmed Stimulus) was omitted and tape-rating and empathy training ‘1!!! "‘91-: I were substit-l intensive :12; H c ' ' _ ~ On .ac111::t-‘ with a client TLRV+D u; _ 2 n4,. ~ “Shane: tC c. Kathleen Rosine Scharf were substituted for this phase of training. Thus the intensive modified IPR training model included a lecture on facilitative conditions in the helping process, tape- rating, empathy training, role-playing, videotaped feed- back, client, counselor, and mutual recall, and interview with a client. Thirty-five undergraduate "helpers" were randomly assigned to one of the two experimental training programs --the intensive modified IPR process (ITM-I) or the in- tensive modified communication skills training (ITM-II). Two comparison groups were also used in the analysis of helper behavior. The training comparison group (ETM) included 22 undergraduate resident assistants who were trained in helping behaviors over a six-month period of time. A second comparison group (PCG) included pro- fessional counselors employed by a university counseling center. Following training, two samples of helper behavior were collected from all groups. An audiotaped interview with a student and a paper-and-pencil test of affective Sensitivity were collected two weeks after training in all four treatment groups——ITM—I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG. JEight weeks later, an audiotaped interview with a student a1'16. the test of affective sensitivity were again collected 1J1 both of the experimental treatment groups--ITM-I and I'I‘M-II--and in the professional counselor group—-PCG. pr These sar;-e.— *‘Hra‘; “I.“w ads-v blonnbtnu ‘ V .._t i. .l'v'lty S" , L‘Q‘lh “~wer's ' . a2» 1 *4. L 53: 7', #:3ic .n testep Kathleen Rosine Scharf These samples were used to assess the levels of helper functioning in each of the groups used in this study. Six criterion measures were used to evaluate the helper's interview behavior. Carkhuff's Empathic Under— standing Scale was used to assess helper empathic re- sponses to student concerns during the audiotaped inter- view. Kagan's Counselor Verbal Response Scale was used to measure the helper's ability to use affective, under- standing, specific, and exploratory responses to student communications during the interview process. The composite CVRS score and the CVRS subscores--CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and CVRS-S-—were used in the analyses of helper interview behavior. An additional measure--Kagan's Affective Sensi— tivity Scale was used as a paper—and-pencil test of the helper's ability to judge what a client, seen in a video- taped counseling session, was feeling about himself and about his counselor. Thus, the overall design used in this study incor— porated two schemes. A posttest design was used to test theeeffectiveness of training on helper behavior between theIfOur comparison groups--the intensive modified IPR tnaining, the intensive modified communication skills Paradigm, the extensive IPR training experience, and the Enofessional counselors group. A post-delayed posttest design tested the effectiveness of training between the two experimental treatment groups--the intensive modified 'PR training «A‘- wv— A.» . cardulj..;'-a.. A per; P of t»; l w... n 0..“- test the one: two experirer. G t 9 risk C: hlpothesls “ “S“: 0'23- Signiflcantl. rem le‘lels. EESt the diff Kathleen Rosine Scharf IPR training, the intensive modified communication skills paradigm--and the professional counselors group over a period of two months. A multivariate analysis of variance was used to test the overall differences in group means between the two experimental training paradigms for undergraduate paraprofessional helpers (ITM-I, ITM-II) and two compari- sons (ETM, PCG). The null hypothesis, that there would be no differences between treatment levels on all vari- ables, was rejected in the multivariate case (p < .10). The overall alpha level was set at .10 to compensate for the risk of a Type II error--false acceptance of the hypothesis. In the univariate case, the means on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables were shown to be significantly different (p < .10) across the four treat- ment levels. Scheffé post hoc comparisons were used to test the differences between the four treatment levels-- ITM-I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG-—on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables. Significant differences (p < .10) were found between the ITM-II and ETM groups on the CVRS-A and between the ITM-II and PCG groups on the CVRS-U and CVRS-E variables. Significant differences were not de- tected between the ITM-I and ITM-II groups, between the lint-I and ETM groups, or between the ITM-I and PCG groups CHI all six of the variables used in this analysis. Signifi- cant differences were also not detected between the ITM-II “9‘1 cf the two 5-,. It) Kathleen Rosine Scharf and PCG groups on the CVRS-A and EU variables. Nor were significant differences detected between the ITM-II and ETM groups on the CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables. A repeated measures analysis of variance was used to test the differences within and between the group means of the two experimental training groups--ITM-I, ITM-II-- and one comparison group--PCG. The null hypothesis, that there would be no difference in the means of the three groups on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables-~collectively or individually—-across the testing periods, failed to be rejected. The hypothesis testing the TAM and TA inter- actions also failed to be rejected. Though not hypothe- sized in this analysis, a treatment main effect was found (p < .05). Significant differences between both the experimental groups--ITM-I, ITM-II--and the professional counselors group were evidenced in the repeated measures analysis. The multivariate analysis and Scheffé post hoc comparisons suggested that significant differences in helper behavior existed in the following areas. The pro- fessional counselors used significantly more understanding and exploratory responses to student concerns during an interview than did the undergraduates trained intensively in.communication skills. Differences were not detected in interview behavior or levels of affective sensitivity between the undergraduates trained intensively in IPR and EIS r 5 fi" Q '7‘ -v A - LA.¢\4 C u H‘ HIC .A . rf‘" '- t¢oVDC ,. o. .a- . (a r». Man .0 a C ova cbabilit Iv Us e i «A 0 h :1 SEC etude that S '~rejecti A . tCC‘Jr l aSQV\~‘ Kathleen Rosine Scharf those helpers trained extensively in IPR. There also seemed to be no differences in interview behavior and levels of affective sensitivity between the professional counselors and those helpers trained intensively in IPR. Yet the repeated measures analysis suggested that the pro- fessional counselors functioned at higher levels of em- pathic understanding, affective sensitivity, and Counselor Verbal Response dimensions than either of the experimental trainee groups-~those helpers trained intensively in IPR and those trainees in the communication skills paradigm. The repeated measures results differed with the multi- variate analysis. Traditionally, caution is observed in interpreting the acceptance of a null hypothesis. The probability of a Type II error--false acceptance--could occur in such "acceptance." It is considered safer to assume that statistical inference delineates the conditions of rejecting a hypothesis or of failing to reject a hypothesis rather than suggesting the acceptance of a null hypothesis. in Part Departing TRAINING OF RESIDENT ASSISTANTS AND PEER GROUP MEMBERS IN THE COMMUNICATION INTERACTIONAL PROCESS SKILLS OF EMPATHIC UNDERSTANDING OF STUDENT FEELING AND STUDENT DEPTH OF SELF-EXPLORATION BY Kathleen Rosine Scharf A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1971 fl , AA ' t Rt, “(I 'V wby..rl~:“ N) “ Vim. - 11,. ; Pr" WISI‘ AAA¢~Q~.\ d\\., ‘ 1972 CO Copyright by KATHLEEN ROSINE SCHARF 1972. DED ICAT ION To my supervisors, Marilyn MacKenzie Gordon Hall John Powell Bill Kell and Cecil Williams, because they cared enough to help me explore and grow. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENT S My journey has been a long one and yet it is just a beginning. Along that path of growth, many people reached out to help me. I cannot thank them all but I would like to mention those who have been with me this year. --To Norm Kagan, my chairman, for his ingenuity and enthusiasm. --To Sam Plyler, for his deep friendship and under— standing. --To Cecil and Bette Williams, for their continuing friendship and encouragement through my years at MSU. --To Andy Porter, for his moral support and his belief that I could learn and understand. --To Bill Kell, for his reaching out to me in my growth as a person and as a therapist. --To Bob Dendy and Jim Archer, for struggling through this dissertation with me. --To Tom Spierling, for his understanding in my deep- est moments of doubt. iii --To Leslie and Barry Wolfe, for their creativity, understanding, friendship, and comfort during these last few months. --To Mary Heiserman, for her comradeship and company through the graduate program. -—To my trainers, Bob Van Noord, Tom Zarle, Jim Archer, Roger Landvoy, Greg Rathjen, and Tom Spierling, for their willingness to give of them- selves and warm friendship. --To my raters, Bob Van Noord, Karen Rowe, and Dave Cabush, for their patience and special friendship. --To Paul Schauble, Al Grzegorek, and Dick Hark, for being themselves. --To Bob Wilson, for his guidance, encouragement, and insight. --To John, Bev, John, and Jeff Powell, for sharing themselves and their home with me. --To Marilyn Wendland, for her caring and support. --To Liz Monroe and Dale Cook, for their concern and loyalty. --To my clients and supervisees who taught me about growing. --To the staff of Holmes Hall, especially Judd Carlberg, and the staff of Lyman Briggs College for their enthusiasm, support, and cooperation. iv '1’! Ill‘llll'l’l‘ nl' --To the Resident Assistants of Holmes Hall and the Peer Group Members of Lyman Briggs College for their willingness to help with this study. --To my family who grew to understand why I wanted to be me. TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Page I. THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . . . . 1 General Statement . . '. . . . . . 1 Specific Problem. . . . . . . . . 2 Definition of Terms. . . . . . . . 3 Need for Research . . . . . . . . 5 Delimitations. . . . . . . . . . 7 Theory . . . . . . . . . . 9 Assumptions and Review of Research Which Supports Assumptions. . . . . . . 18 Specific Objectives. . .‘ . . . . . 25 Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . . 26 Overview of the Training Programs . . . 28 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . 29 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE. . . . . . . 31 Use of Paraprofessionals . . . . . . 32 Carkhuff Training Paradigm . . . . . 42 Kagan and His Associates . . . . . . 54 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . 65 III. DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . 66 Design . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 Population. . . . . . . . . . . 72 Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 Experimental Procedures . . . . . . 78 Day 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 Day 2 and Day 3 . . . . . . . . . 82 Day 4 and 5 . . . . . . . . . . 88 Instrumentation . . .. . . . . . . 90 Collection of Data . . . . . . . 99 Preparation of the Audiotapes for Rating. 101 Reliability of Judges' Ratings . . . . 102 Analysis of the Data . . . . . . . 105 Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . . 107 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . 108 vi Chapter IV. ANALYSIS OF THE RESULTS . . . . . . . Hypotheses Related to the Between Treatment Groups Design. . . . . Hypotheses of the Within Treatment Groups Design . . . . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . V. SUMMARY, DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS. . . . sumary O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Discussion. . . . . . . . . . Observations and Speculations . . . . Implications for Future Research . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES Appendix A. The Empathic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes Scale and Rating Form. . . . B. The IPR Counselor Verbal Response Scale and Rating Form . . . . . . . . . . C. Helpee Self-Exploration in Interpersonal Processes Scale . . . . . . . . . D. Role and Function of the Inquirer. . . . E. Trainer's Manual-—ITM-I Training Procedures F. Trainer's Manual—-ITM—II Training Procedures. . . . . . . . . . (3. Memo to Professional Counselors . . . . . Reliability Raw Data . . . . . . . . 13. The Affective Sensitivity Scale . . . . vii Page 110 110 118 126 130 130 140 146 150 152 158 162 172 175 177 183 188 189 190 3.4. LIST OF TABLES Helper Tasks, Goals, Training Exercises in ITM-I and in ITM-II o o o o o o o 0 Description of Volunteers. . . . . Summary of the Experimental Procedures . . Kuder-Richardson Formula 20 Reliability Coefficients and Other Related Data for Seven Sample Groups . . . . . . . . Overview of Three Studies Designed to Determine Interrater Reliability on the CVRS O O O O O O O O O I C O 0 Average Tape and Item Interrater Reliability Coefficients for Each Dimension of the CVRS on Three Separate Studies . . . . Interrater Reliabilities--CVRS . . . . . Mean Scores of Six Counselors at Various Levels of Training on Each of the Dimensions of the CVRS . . . . . . . ANOVA Table and Computation of Reliability of Ratings on the CVRS (Counselor Verbal Response Scale) . . . . . . . . . ANOVA Table and Computation of Reliability of Ratings on the E Scale . . . . . . Raw Data--Group and Cell Means for the Multivariate ANOVA . . . . . . . . Univariate F Test Results. . . . . . . Differences in the Group Means on the CVRS-A O O O O O O O O O O O 0 viii Page 30 76 80 92 96 97 98 99 104 104 112 113 114 4.11, 4.4. H.l. Table 4.4. Differences in the CVRS-U O O O 0 Differences in the CVRS-E O O O 0 Differences in the Group Means on the EU Raw Data for Three Across Times. . Transformed Scores Group Means on the Group Means on the Comparison Groups ANOVA Table of Transformed Scores . Post-Hoc Comparisons of the T Main Effect. Summary of the Hypotheses and Results Reliability Raw Data. . . . . . ix Page 115 116 117 120 121 123 125 129 189 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 3.1. Experimental Design, Three Non-equivalent Samples . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 3.2. Analysis Between Groups, Treatments, Measures. . . . . . . . . . . . 69 3.3. Analysis Within Groups, Treatments, Measures, Administrations . . . . . . 71 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM General Statement This study is an attempt to contribute to the solution of the general problem of improving mental health among students on a college campus. More specifically, this study will compare the effectiveness of two different approaches to the training of undergraduate paraprofes- sional mental health workers or "helpers." The investi- gation will attempt to determine the kinds of trainee behavior evidenced following an intensive 40-hour train- ing experience conducted during five consecutive days in each of the two approaches. In the analysis of helper behavior following training, two comparison groups are used. One comparison group includes undergraduates who were trained extensively in a 38-hour IPR program which was conducted over a six-month period of time. A second comparison group includes professional trained counselors who are currently employed by a university counseling center. Specific Problem This investigation is of the training of under—" graduate helpers in working with peer vocational and personal needs in the university environment. On the Michigan State University campus, resident assistants and academic aides--such as Peer Group members--are students employed to be helpful to their peers. Resident assistants are student personnel who function as resource persons, friends, listeners, and helpers to students living in the residence halls. Peer Group members are concerned with promoting academic growth within the Lyman Briggs resi- dential college. Their activities include identification ‘of peer academic problems and referral of students to apprOpriate college staff; they also conduct groups, tutor students, and lead seminars. These two groups of student helpers are the sources of lay mental health workers to be trained to have an even greater impact on peer growth in the university environment. This study will investigate two training procedures, each of which is designed to increase the "interpersonal communication skills" of student helpers. These inter- personal communication skills include helper empathic understanding as measured by Carkhuff's Empathic Under- standing in Interpersonal Processes (see Appendix A) and the helper's ability to use affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory responses (Kagan's IPR Counselor n! U] ' fi Verbal Response Scale--see Appendix B). A secondary skill, student self-eXploration as measured by Carkhuff's Helpee Self-Exploration in Interpersonal Processes (see Appendix C), is used during training but not as an outcome measure . Definition of Terms In this investigation special terms are defined as follows. Affective Sensitivity Scale.-—A scale which pur— ports to measure affective sensitivity as a test of empathy (Kagan, Krathwohl, Goldberg, Campbell, Schauble, Greenberg, Danish, Resnikoff, Bowes, & Bondy, 1967). This multiple choice test requires the student helper to judge what the client, seen in a videotaped counseling session, is feeling about himself and about the counselor. Communication Skills Training.--An approach to the training of helpers. This five—step procedure was designed by Carkhuff and his associates to " . . . cog- nitively and experientially . . . " (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965a, 1965b) train helpers in four dimensions of helper behavior--empathy, regard, concreteness, and genuineness-- and one dimension of helpee behavior--exploration of feelings. Counselor Verbal Response Scale.--A scale which describes a helper's response to client communication in terms of four dimensions: affective—cognitive; under- standing-non-understanding; specific-non-specific; and exploratory-non-exploratory. These dimensions represent aspects of helper behavior and seem to be associated with the progress of the client. Affective responses generally make references to the emotions, feelings, or fears of the client. Understanding responses communicate to the stu- dent that the helper understands or is trying to under- stand what the student is expressing, both verbally and non-verbally. Specific responses are honest statements by the helper of the core concerns being presented either explicitly or implicitly, verbally or non-verbally by the student. EXploratory responses indicate whether the helper encourages or permits the student to eXplore his concerns (see Appendix B). Empathic Understanding.--Helper sensitivity to current feelings as evidenced by his verbal facility to communicate this understanding to a student in a language attuned to the peer's current feelings (Carkhuff, 1969b). Helper Behavior.--The helper's verbal responses to student statements of concern. These helper responses are assumed to be a measure of the communications of the helper during the interview. Inguirer.--The third person in a helper-student relationship. The inquirer conducts the recall session for the helper and/or the student. The role and function of the inquirer is to act as a neutral stimulus who facilitates examination of the underlying dynamics of an interaction. The inquirer tries to avoid establishing a new relationship with the person being recalled by focusing attention on the video playback rather than on himself. Interpersonal Process Recall.--A series of tasks designed to intensify and accelerate counselor and/or helper training. IPR uses video playback of an interview session in order to stimulate recall of the dynamics under- lying a helper-student interaction. Stimulated Recall Session.--A phase in the IPR procedure in which the videotape of an interview session is played back and an inquirer encourages the trainee to examine the underlying dynamics of an interaction. Training Program.—-A structured sequence of experiential tasks between a group of trainees and a trainer. These tasks are designed to promote trainee growth in facilitative behaviors. Need for Research The need for increased contact and communication between mental health workers--such as teachers, counselors, administrators, and social workers--and the population in general has been discussed by several authors (Brown, 1965; Crakhuff & Truax, 1965a, 1965b, 1966; Harvey, 1964; Magoon & Golann, 1966; Rioch, Elkes, Flint, Usdansky, Newman, & Silber, 1963; Schmidt, 1968; Zunker & Brown, 1966). Unrest in urban communities and on college campuses has emphasized this need. Yet identity needs and needs for interpersonal closeness still seem to be a crucial concern of the pOpulation in general. For example, students are currently vocal about defining their rights, their roles, and the kinds of impact they might have on the university as a whole. Much of this protest is legitimate and has led to positive changes, but some of the protest seems to be the result of interpersonal frustration which may be partially attributable to a breakdown in effective communication in the university community. Several investigators have suggested the use of lay personnel in promoting mental health and academic— vocational adjustment (Brown, 1965; Truax & Carkhuff, 1965a, 1965b; Rioch, gt 31., 1963; Schlossberg, 1966; Zunker & Brown, 1966). Conflict about the role of lay professionals has focused on the use of such individuals as aides or assistants only, as opposed to the use of lay persons in the direct counseling or interview process. Carkhuff (1968) and Schlossberg (1966) have both defined five areas in which lay helpers could be useful. They can be effective in working with peer vocational and per- sonal needs in the environment, in fulfilling some of the manpower needs of the helping professions, in increasing the effectiveness of services being offered, in assisting peer develOpment of a sense of identity, and in helping maximize peer potential. Delimitations Two modified training programs will be investi- gated--one version of Kagan's IPR process and of Carkhuff's communication skills training. The IPR process usually includes a four-step process-~a lecture on facilitative conditions; simulated confrontation recall (filmed stimu- lus); counselor, client, and mutual recall; and individual interviews. In this study, simulation confrontation recall will be deleted. The lecture on facilitative conditions will omit the CVRS scale and use Carkhuff's E and EX scales instead. Thus the CVRS could be used as an un- biased measure across all groups following training. Carkhuff's scheme includes a five-step process in the facilitative conditions of helper empathic understanding, regard, genuineness, and concreteness, and helpee self- exploration. This study will use the same five—step process, but will focus only on the facilitative dimensions of helper empathic understanding and helpee self- exploration. An attempt will be made to assess facilitative conditions offered by professional counselors to students. Students interviewed by the counselors and trainees are similar. No long-term therapy interviews will be included in the sample; all interviews are structured as a one-time only meeting. Instructions to the clients are, "If you feel the person whom you will be seeing is helpful, please feel free to discuss a personal concern." Instructions to the trainees and counselors were, "Relate to the person you will be seeing as you would ordinarily do in order to be helpful to him." These instructions will be used to help control the structure of the interview. Post and delayed-post measures will be collected. Delayed-post measures, measuring retention of helper be- haviors, will be taken to study trainee behavior over time. No premeasures on the CVRS and B will be taken since random assignment of trainees to groups and treatments is employed. No assessment will be made of supervisor behavior during training. Supervisors will be randomly assigned to treatment and then trained independently. These pre— cautions will be taken to prevent some sources of super- visor bias. The investigator will serve as a program coordinator and will daily brief each trainer. This might partially control for deviation from the training procedures. All findings in this study must be interpreted within the confines of the above delimitations. Theory The theoretical bases for each of the training models in this study are described here. Basic to the helping process is the interaction between the helper and the student being helped. The dynamics of that inter- action seem crucial to the kinds of growth which may occur as the result of that contact. Three variables need consideration—-the helper, the student being helped, and the helper-student relationship. An underlying assumption in discussing these variables is that the helper-student interaction involves relatively normal behavior--behavior essential to the develOpmental process of a student, as he learns how to meet his needs within his current environ— ment, and as he seeks to know himself and others. The Helper The helper brings a dynamic personal Gestalt to the helping relationship (Kell & Mueller, 1966). He brings a whole history of past relationships, experiences, and ways he has learned to c0pe with stresses, anxieties, and developmental problems. The helper also has a "pre- sent." This "present" includes his current feelings, fears, expectations of others, concerns, hostilities, and satisfactions or dissatisfactions with his interpersonal relationships. 10 The Student The student likewise adds his own personality con- figuration to the helper-student interaction (Kell & Mueller, 1966). In asking for assistance, the student is experiencing some degree of stress about a concern. This concern may be the result of a new experience, a reflection of a long history of such conflict, or a kind of inter- personal impasse in the student's process of coping with his environment. The student also has his own anxieties, fears, feelings, c0ping behaviors, hostilities, and experi- ences with interpersonal relationships. The Helper-Student Relationship The third variable in this theoretical construct is the reciprocal impact of the helper and student—-the kind of impact the helper has on the student and the kinds of responses the student elicits from the helper. In the context of the interaction, the helper can be verbally and non-verbally responding to the aspects of the student's behavior. This may be more related at times to the helper's Gestalt than to the student's field of experience. Simi- larly, during the interview the student may experience Other concerns in addition to the one he is expressing; and these concerns may be the result of the dynamics of the interaction. 11 Kagan and Schauble (1969) observed that while a counselor and a client are discussing experiences outside the counseling relationship, they can be at the same time experiencing conflicting feelings about the relationship itself. Feelings typically evidenced in their studies were the client's concern that the counselor might hurt or re— ject him, the client's concern that the counselor might make an affectionate, dependent, or seductive approach toward him, the client's concern that his own hostile feel- ings toward the counselor might emerge, and the client's concern that his own affectionate, dependent, or seductive impulses toward the counselor might be acted out. Danish and Kagan (1969) believe that these conflictual feelings exist in most interpersonal relationships. The above authors focus on the client half of the interaction. Grzegorek (1971) and Spivak (1970), on the other hand, both assumed that counselors needed to look at these factors in themselves. It may be possible that the counselor is also experiencing his own concerns about these four areas. For instance, the counselor may be concerned about hurting or rejecting the client. The counselor may be concerned that the client will make an affectionate, dependent, or seductive approach toward him. The counselor may be wondering about his own hostile feelings toward the client; and the counselor may be frightened about his own affectionate, dependent, or seductive impulses being acted out with the client. 12 Developmental Tasks It is important to consider the kinds of develop- mental tasks that a helper needs to learn in order to be helpful to another person in stress. Kagan's and Cark— huff's theoretical models differ in their consideration of counselor and client develOpmental tasks in the helping process. Kagan and his associates (1967) delineated four tasks that a counselor needed to learn in order to become effective in his interpersonal communications. These tasks were defined as follows. 1. The counselor needs to become aware of and sensi- tive to the elements of effective communication. The counselor needs to have some cognitive notions or theories about the underlying elements of effec- tive interpersonal communication. In some ways this can be a model of what constitutes the pro- cess of being helpful. Counselor behaviors, then, are an operational expression of this model. Several researchers (Kagan, et 31., 1967; Rogers, 1957; Carkhuff, 1966; Truax, Carkhuff, & Douds, 1964) have identified several elements of com- munication which may be crucial in effective interpersonal encounters. 2. The counselor needs to become aware of and sensi- tive to his own feelings. The counselor must first understand and own his feelings, so that he 13 can eventually understand how the client experi— ences feelings. In addition, the counselor must recognize how he communicates his feelings to the client. He may come to recognize how these are communicated verbally or non—verbally, subtly or overtly. The counselor must become aware of and sensitive to the client's communication. In order to be helpful, the counselor needs to become aware of the client's feelings and experiences, from the context of the client's Gestalt (Rogers, 1957). At times, this skill is defined as accurate empathy (Rogers, 1957; Truax & Carkhuff, 1967). Some re- searchers have posited this skill as a central ingredient in the helping process. Truax and Carkhuff (1967) suggest that "the therapist's ability to perceive and communicate, accurately and with sensitivity, the feelings of the patient and the meaning of those feelings" (p. 284) is central to the helping process. The counselor must be aware of the bilateral nature of the counseling interaction. It is important that the counselor understands that the counselor and client have a reciprocal impact upon each other. The counselor serves as a stimulus to the client during the interview. 14 Likewise the client elicits counselor behaviors during the session. Kell and Mueller (1966) ex- plain that in order to understand the counseling process, the counselor needs to examine his own previous behaviors which might have triggered the client's thoughts, feelings, and affective changes. Also the interaction between the counselor and the client in the current relationship affects the choice and sequence of the content in the inter— view (Kell & Mueller, 1966). Psychoanalytic theory presents the notions of transference and countertransference as a way of conceptualizing the bilateral nature of the therapeutic interaction. Transference is defined as the " . . . emotional reaction of the patient to the therapist in which he directs his impulses and attitudes toward the therapist" (Patterson, 1966, p. 321). The psycho- analytic therapist must be aware of the dynamics of the client's transference. In addition, the therapist needs to be cognizant of his own counter- transference attitudes; and he must be able to control them, and to substitute appropriate atti- tudes toward the client. The psychoanalytic pro- cess then consists of the develOpment of the transference and its resolution between the therapist and the client (Patterson, 1966). 15 Finally, dimensions of the counselor-client interaction may be related to the dimensions of the client's interpersonal encounters outside of the interview. In their conceptual model, Truax and Carkhuff (1967) explain that the counselor needs to learn and to communicate at minimally facilitative levels four helper behaviors or conditions. The counselor needs to be at least a minimally effective communicator of empathic understanding, respect, concreteness, and genuineness. Empathic understanding is defined as helper sensitivity to current feelings as evidenced by his verbal facility to communicate this understanding to a student in a language attuned to the client's current feelings. The counselor communicates respect when he warmly accepts the client's experience as part of that person, without imposing con- ditions or evaluations. The counselor is genuine when he is freely and deeply himself in the helping relationship. His responses are sincere rather than phony; these re- sponses express the counselor's real feelings rather than his defensive feelings. Lastly, counselor concreteness is defined as the helper's ability to directly discuss specific feelings and experiences related to the client's concerns. Truax and Carkhuff (1967) explain that the levels of these helping behaviors communicated by the counselor to the client during the helping interaction 16 affect the level of client behavior in that process. These authors consider that a client who stays at low levels of self-exploration may be the result of a counselor who is functioning at low levels of facilitative behaviors. Thus, progress or deterioration in the helping process can be related to the levels of counselor dimen- sions offered in the counseling relationship. Important client tasks in the helping process have also been investigated. Rogers (1957) explains that it the counselor provides six basic conditions in the thera- peutic relationship, E222 constructive personality change in the client can occur. Truax and Carkhuff (1967) con- sider one client behavior in the therapeutic process-- client self—exploration. They prOpose that at level five of client self-eXploration, "the person is fully and actively focusing upon himself and exploring himself and his world. He actively and spontaneously engages in an inward probing to newly discovered feelings and experi- ences about himself" (see Appendix C--the Ex Scale). These investigators sketchily describe client develop- mental tasks. Kagan and his associates (1967) discuss four client developmental tasks which are presumed to be neces- sary if growth is to occur in the helping process. The client owns his discomfort; he admits his own anxiety and unhappiness. He begins to see that his feelings are tied to 17 concerns, peOple, fears, or other significant sources. He "owns" and accepts his anxiety. The client commits himself to change; he wants to change, says so, and c00perates with the counselor. The client not only faces his problem directly but also is willing to face the consequences of changing. The client begins to differenti- ate stimulii; he begins to realize that peOple and events are individual and he learns to respond to them as such. The fourth task focuses on the client behaving differently during the counseling interview. He also reports noticing behavior changes in his everyday life. The client explores his problems more reasonably and redefines his relationship with the counselor. He engages in new behavior with the counselor and describes to the counselor new behaviors in his everyday life. These researchers have focused primarily on the helping process as it occurs in therapy or counseling. Some investigators suggest that a helping process can also occur between a lay professional and a client. This investigation considers the helping process as it might occur between students on a college campus. Several sources of student trainees can be identified within the university. This study questions what training pro- cedures could be used to increase effective helper be— haviors, what kinds of growth can occur following an intense training program of short duration, and whether these behaviors can be maintained over a period of time. 18 Assumptions and Review of Research Which Supports Assumptions In the context of this study, the university is identified as an agent of student academic and personal- social growth. Educational institutions primarily struc- ture programs for the develOpment of the student's in- tellectual skills. Yet the student's personal and social development also occurs within that setting and is in- creasingly of concern to university faculty. Teacher- student, administrator-student, counselor-student, student- student interactions and relationships thus have the potential of fostering or deterring this postadolescent growth (Carkhuff, 1966; Rogers, 1957). Also, student needs for identity, freedom, self-eXploration, self- direction, dependence-independence, self—realization, creativity, social sensitivity, and intellectual skills all impinge on the structure, goals, atmosphere, and staff of the university (Carkhuff, 1966). For student growth and adjustment to occur, then,effective com— munication between staff and students is necessary. This study assumes that elements of minimally effective communication can be identified. Researchers have posited that elements of good communication can be identified and that the level of these conditions can have either constructive or deteriorative consequences (Carkhuff & Truax, 1966). Interpersonal learning occurs in such relationships as parent-child, teacher-student, 19 and counselor-client. Success or failure in interpersonal learning can be related to the type and quality of certain dimensions in the interaction. Rogers (1957) delineated six conditions which he posited as necessary in a counsel- ing relationship. Two people must be aware of each other; ,a__-_..._a———--—wo—- ‘—~__ _.... — ..h_. they must be in psychological contact with each other. ._..__,_' H...— ‘_—— .———_~____ a The client is in a state of anxiety and the counselor is ‘ ‘ M“. -____ .- “6% —~—‘__ congruent or integrated in the relationsh1p. The counselor m... .,_“ “*‘fi experiences acceptance of the client (unconditional posi- ‘h-_ “mm ‘mm_k _.. .fl. —.—_- ___, w ——.__.. __ _ ' ‘w Mm wuo—a—Oflfi” F" tive regard) as well as an empathic understanding of the M M\‘ client's internal frame of reference. He endeavors to 11.——r It communicate this understanding to the client. The com- munication of the counselor's empathy and acceptance must be minimally achieved. In addition, three helper dimensions and one client characteristic have been shown to be related to the constructive growth of the client in a variety of settings, regardless of orientation (Carkhuff & Truax, 1966; Truax & Carkhuff, 1967). These variables include the helper's empathic understanding; the helper's com- munication of respect; the helper's genuineness; and the client's ability and willingness to eXplore his own feel- ings, thoughts, and fears. Counselor and client behaviors in effective inter- personal communication have been identified by Kagan and his associates (Kagan, gt gt., 1967; Kagan, Schauble, 20 Resnikoff, Danish, & Krathwohl, 1969; Kagan & Schauble, 1969). Effective counselor verbal behaviors are defined in terms of various effective modes of response. The counselor makes reference to or encourages some affective or feeling aspect of the client's communication--affective response. The counselor is able to convey to the client his awareness of and sensitivity to the client's feelings and concerns--understanding response. The counselor deals with the core of the client's concern and is honest in the labels he uses--specific response. The counselor also encourages the client to be the more active of the two in exploring his feelings; and the counselor provides an opportunity for the client to do so--exploratory response. Specific client variables are also associated with client change and growth. The client owns his dis- comfort; he commits himself to change; he differentiates stimuli; and the client behaves differently with his counselor. Robinson (1955) focused on counselor and client response dimensions which played a crucial role in the interactional learning process. Counselor variables in- cluded the counselor's attitude of acceptance of the client, his response to the core of the client's feeling, his shared responsibility with the client for the progress of the interview, and the degree to which the counselor's remarks tended to probe client statements. Dimensions of 21 client behavior were the degree of the client's motivation, the stage the client had reached in thinking about his problem, the role the client expected the counselor to play, the type of problem with which the client entered counseling, and client proclivities, such as dependence, aggression, and transference. Research also supports the assumption that elements of effective communication can be learned and incorporated within an individual's behavioral repertoire. Paradigms for training persons in effective interpersonal skills usually have been sketchily reported in the literature, and the elements of interpersonal communication have been subjected to much controversy. Kagan and his associates (1967) described four major developmental tasks which face a counselor in learning about the process of effective interpersonal communication. According to this description the trainee becomes increasingly aware of the elements of good counseling; and the dimensions of counselor behaviors which characterize effective counseling are defined. The trainee becomes sensitive to, and understands, a greater amount of client communication. He also becomes aware of and sensitive to his own feelings during the counseling session. Finally the helper becomes sensitive to the bilateral or reciprocal nature of the counseling inter- action; he perceives ways in which the dynamics of both the counselor and the counseled may serve to either expedite or inhibit the relationship. ”I”: .Jr ”HH- r.avb.. .. r“; as. 22 Kagan's Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) tech- niques have been used to stimulate recall of the underlying dynamics involved in such interpersonal interactions as those between counselor and client and teacher and learner (Kagan & Schauble, 1969). The IPR process in- volved three steps. A counselor and client first con- ducted an interview which was videotaped and, immediately after the interview, the counselor left the room and an "inquirer" entered the room. Then the inquirer and the client played back the interview in order to discuss recalled feelings or thoughts (Kagan, gt gt., 1967; Kagan & Schauble, 1969). Significant differences were found in the use of IPR in counselor education and in the acceler- ation of client growth in counseling (Kagan, gt gt., 1967; Schauble, 1970; Spivack, 1970; Grzegorek, 1971). In another training paradigm, a five-stage training process in effective growth—producing dimensions was de- lineated (Truax & Carkhuff, 1965a, 1967b). In this train- ing process the trainees were didactically taught thera- peutic dimensions of helper empathic understanding (E), helper respect (R), helper genuineness (G), and client self-exploration (Ex). Trainees then practiced discrimi- nation of five levels of each of the four dimensions. During empathy training, the trainee listened to a client's statements and then wrote a response in terms <3f both the feeling and the content of the communication. Experiential role-playing exercises enabled the trainees 23 to practice and evaluate their own facilitative behaviors, using research dimensions as measures. Trainees then con- ducted initial interviews with a client. Following the initial interview, the trainee's behaviors were rated and feedback about those behaviors was provided. Research evidence indicated that lay persons and graduate students could be trained to function at minimally facilitative levels in E, R, and G (Carkhuff, 1968). Length of the training programs ranged from twenty hours to one year (Berenson, Carkhuff, & Myrus, 1966; Carkhuff, 1968; Carkhuff & Truax, 1965b). After about 100 hours of training, lay helpers and graduate students functioned at levels of facilitative conditions comparable to experi— enced professionals (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965b). Following 16 hours of training in the research dimensions, volunteer college students demonstrated significantly greater im— provement than did their controls, as measured by rated taped interviews, interviewee reports, self—reports, and reports of significant others (Berenson, Carkhuff, & Myrus, 1966). Fourth semester rehabilitation counselor trainees functioned at significantly higher levels of communication than both first semester rehabilitation students and fourth semester philOSOphy students (Anthony & Carkhuff, 1970). Training programs of academic lay and professional counselors have also been discussed by researchers (Brown, 24 1965; Zunker & Brown, 1966). Following 50 hours of identi— cal training, student counselors were found to be as effective as professional counselors in the criteria of communication of information, impact on counseled stu- dents' academic achievement, and satisfaction ratings of the students counseled (Zunker & Brown, 1966). Thus, it can be assumed that lay personnel can be at least minimally effective facilitators in promoting student growth. There is research evidence to indicate that lay persons can effect significant constructive changes in the clients whom they see (Carkhuff, 1968). Hospitalized neurOpsychiatric patients in lay counselor led groups showed significant improvement, as compared to controls, on such measures as a pre-post Anxiety Scale (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965a). Lay personnel engaged hos- pitalized patients in a depth of interpersonal exploration commensurate with that of experienced therapists and graduate students (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965b). In another study, 30 out of 49 neurOpsychiatric outpatients seen by lay counselors showed some improvement as measured by clinical ratings (Rioch, gt gt., 1963). Freshmen counseled by students showed significant improvements, as compared to controls, on such dimensions as test-retest differential (Study Habits and Attitudes), grades, and quality point ratios (Brown, 1965). Student counselors achieved signifi- cantly better results in promoting freshman growth than professional counselors on certain outcome measures, such _ l 25 as client use of study skills knowledge, client satis— faction, and grade point ratios (Zunker & Brown, 1966). The rationale of this study has thus been based on four assumptions. The university can be identified as an agent of student academic and personal-social growth. The elements of minimally effective communication can be identified. The elements of effective communication can be learned and incorporated within an individual's be- havioral repertoire. Lay personnel can be at least minimally effective facilitators of their peers' growth. This investigation then tests how much growth in helper behavior can be evidenced as the result of an intensive training experience, what kinds of helper growth can be evidenced in each of the two intensive training programs used, and whether helper behavior can be maintained over time. Specific Objectives Training of Resident Assistants and Peer Groupers includes four general goals. In this study the training attempts to make trainees more aware of and sensitive to the feelings and concerns of their peers, while it also seeks to provide trainees with listening and communication skills which facilitate understanding between them and students desiring help. The training programs attempt to offer student helpers effective ways of responding to and behaving with students, and also to give trainees an 26 increased awareness of their role and impact as helpers in the college and residence hall. Specific skills identified in this study as facilitating increased communication and understanding within the milieu include helper empathic understandipg (E) and helper use of affective, understanding, specific, and eXploratory responses (Kagan's CVRS) to student com- munication. Hypotheses This study will compare the effectiveness of both the Kagan and Carkhuff approaches to the training of undergraduate paraprofessional mental health workers or "helpers." Both models seem to be effective training procedures. Thus, one hypothesis in this study predicts that there will be no differences in trainee group levels of Empathic Understanding (E), Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) dimensions, and Affective Sensitivity Scale (ASS) scores between the trainees who receive modified IPR training procedures and those trainees who receive modified communications skill training (Truax-Carkhuff paradigm). Carkhuff (1968) suggests that lay persons can be trained to function at minimally facilitative levels of helPer conditions in relatively short periods of time. ThuS, the second hypothesis predicts that there will be no differences in trainee group levels of Empathic 27 Understanding (E), Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) dimensions, and Affective Sensitivity Scale (ASS) scores between the experimental trainee groups (IPR and/or com— munication skills 40-hour intensive programs) and those trainees who receive approximately the same amount of training in IPR over a six-month period of time--the treatment comparison group. Carkhuff (1968) also proposes that lay helpers can function at levels essentially as high or higher than pro- fessional trainees or counselors. Thus, the third hypothesis predicts that there will be no differences in helper group levels of Empathic Understanding (E), Counse- lor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) dimensions, and Affective Sensitivity Scale (ASS) scores between the experimental trainees (IPR and/or communication skills programs) and the comparison group of trained professional counselors. The fourth hypothesis tests whether helper be- haviors following training remain at the same level in the environment over a period of time. Grzegorek (1971) sug— gests that levels of helper behavior following treatment can be maintained over a period of time following train- ing (four weeks after treatment). Thus the last hypothesis in this study predicts that there will be no differences in the modified IPR and modified communication skills trainees' levels of Empathic Understanding (E), Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) dimensions, and Affective 28 Sensitivity Scale (ASS) scores two weeks after training compared to eight weeks after training. Overview of the Training Programs Two training programs were used in this study. Intensive Training Model I (ITM-I) is the intensive (short-term) modified IPR program. The procedures are fully described in Appendix E. The model for this program was developed by Kagan and his associates (1967). Inten— sive Training Model II (ITM-II) is the intensive (short- term) modified communication skills program. Procedures are fully described in Appendix F. The model for this program was develOped by Carkhuff and his associates (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965a, 1965b). Both training models used four sources of learning --the didactic, the experiential, the role model, and the actual interview process. The didactic mode refers to the direct shaping of behavior. In this process, facilitative behaviors were defined, operationalized, practiced, and rated. The experiential mode included role-playing exer- cises with a trainee—client and with coached clients; these exercises then were used as a type of practicum experience for the trainees. Through the use of the role model, facilitative helper behaviors were modeled by the trainers and were shaped and practiced by the trainees. Interviews with coached and actual clients were used so that the trainees could test their facilitative behaviors in an 29 experience which closely approximated their job experience. Table 1.1 delineates the helper tasks, the goals related to each develOpmental task, and the trainee exercises in both ITM-I and ITM-II which were used to achieve each develOpmental task. Summary This study is designed to assess helper growth evidenced as the result of an intensive training eXperi- ence focused on helper behaviors. Two training models—- intensive modified IPR and intensive modified communication skills training-—were used during the week prior to the fall term of 1970. These two treatments are compared with each other and with two comparison groups. One comparison group includes resident assistants who had been trained in the IPR process over a period of six months. The second comparison group includes professional trained counselors. Carkhuff's Empathic Understanding Scale (E), Kagan's Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) dimensions, and Kagan's Affective Sensitivity Scale (ASS) are used as criterion measures of helper behaviors in all groups. The literature pertinent to paraprofessional training is reviewed in Chapter II. Procedures, method- §periential-accepting (EA) and the cognitive-intellectual (CII)--were based on four counselor developmental tasks. 'Trlis investigator attempted to determine whether trainee learning was more related to affective involvement or to cOgnitive involvement. Pre-, post-, and delayed—post meEasures were collected. Following 80 hours of training, t1Iere were no significant differences between the two 59 groups on affective sensitivity as measured by the ASS. The EA group scored significantly higher than the CI groups on the Empathic Understanding scale and on the understand— ing, specific, and exploratory subscales of the CVRS. This author concluded that the experiential—accepting approach (affective learning) had a significant\overall effect on counselor performance in training. Both the Spivack (1970) and the Grzegorek (1971) studies supported the notion that the IPR process could be eeffectively used with counselor trainees. However, research Imas not considered the efficacy of this training model with puaraprofessional helpers. This study, then, will attempt to tmest the kinds of paraprofessional trainee growth that might 1363 evidenced as the result of an intensive IPR experience. Because of time limitations, the simulation confrontation (ifilms) eXperience will be deleted in this study. Supervision The IPR process has also been used in the super— 'Vfiision of counselor trainees. Kagan and his associates (1.967; Goldberg, 1967) formulated a supervisory experi- elice that was based on the four counselor developmental tlasks. Trainees needed to become aware of the elements of good counseling. Counselor-trainees needed to understand and become sensitive to a greater amount of client com- nIunication, while also becoming sensitive to their own feelings during the counseling session. Finally, these 6O helpers needed to become sensitive to the bilateral feature of the counseling interaction. The IPR tasks of stimulated recall and videotape feedback were used to facilitate trainee learning of these tasks. Trainees in this project interviewed their clients for 30 minutes weekly over a period of six weeks. The traditional supervision group spent 60 minutes after each interview reviewing the audio- taped session with their supervisor. The IPR supervision groups' sequential experience was as follows. Immediately following the first two interviews, a lS-minute client recall and a 45—minute counselor recall were conducted by an inquirer. For the next two sessions, trainees conducted Client recalls for each other. A supervisor conducted 60- minute mutual recalls for the last two interviews. Both Supervisory eXperiences were found to be effective in pro- mOting pre to post trainee changes in interview behavior. But the IPR supervision trainees showed significantly greater gains on the CVRS dimensions than those trainees in the traditional supervision groups. The authors con— c311.1ded that a sequential series of supervisory experiences uSing the IPR methods were effective in training counselors during a practicum. Kagan and Goldberg did not, however, investigate the impact of the supervisor's personality or level of functioning on trainee growth. 61 Acceleration of Client Progress in Therapy Kagan and his associates (1967) observed that the IPR techniques seemed to accelerate the counseling pro- cess. That is, the impasses and plateaus which usually hindered the counseling or therapy process were diminished. Studies were conducted to appraise the accelerating effect of the IPR process on client progress in therapy. Four therapeutic variables were defined as criteria of client growth in the counseling relationship. Client develop— mental tasks included the following. The client owned his discomfort. He admitted the feelings of anxiety or un- happiness and began to specify the sources of his concerns, fears, and discomfort. The client committed himself to The client stayed with the problem, confronted change. it, and tried to work out other coping behaviors. The gglient differentiated stimulii. That is, the client began t1) differentiate his own emotions and accurately perceived tile emotions of others and their reactions to him. Finally, .Efile client behaved differently with his counselor. He re- dGafined his relationship with others and with the counselor. Kagan and Schauble (1969) and Danish and Kagan (1.969) reported positive initial findings concerning the ‘JSSe of affect stimulation with clients in therapy. In t1'lese studies clients were exposed to various kinds and (ifiigrees of emotional situations on a film. The client's 1Te‘actions to these stimulus films were videotaped. After 62 each exposure, a counselor and the client reviewed the videotaped behavior. Thus the client's videotaped be- havior became the focus for a personal counseling session. These initial studies suggested that affective simulation within the IPR process might accelerate a client's ability to perceive, differentiate, and gain insight into his reactions. The use of stimulated recall in therapy has also been investigated. Experimentation in the therapy process had been structured in the following manner. A counselor and a client conducted a counseling interview which was videotaped. Immediately following the interview, the counselor and client recalled the interaction separately ‘with an inquirer. Thus, both the counselor and the client examined the interpersonal communication process. Kagan, Krathwohl, and Miller (1963) reported in their initial studies that the use of these procedures with Clients and counselors enabled these subjects to reveal at length and in depth much of their subtle or semi- mo cnmccmum muomnnsm mo umnEdz mHmEmm concumcoflmunmpsm mesouw mHmEmm cm>wm MOM memo omumamm umsuo can mucmfloflmmmoo muflaflnmwamm om mHsEuom QOmpumconuumcsm m.m OHQMB ? 93 subject's sensitivity and the subject's ASS score. In a predictive validity study, Kagan (1967) reported a corre- lation of .49 between initial ASS scores in a year long NDEA institute and later peer ratings of effectiveness. In construct validity investigations, groups of counselor trainees increased significantly (at the .025 and .005 levels) during a pretest to posttest training period of six months (Kagan, pp pp., 1967). The ASS was used in this study to measure trainee growth in affective sensitivity as the result of a train— ing experience. The ASS was also administered to subjects in the comparative groups. The Empathic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes II (E) Scale This scale was developed by Carkhuff from Truax's Accurate Empathy Scale. This revised five-point scale attempted to measure levels of helper empathic understand- ing during live observations or tape recordings of counsel— ing or therapy interviews. Empathic Understanding was defined as the helper's sensitivity to current feelings of a client and his verbal ability to communicate this understanding in a language attuned to a client's current feelings. The five levels of empathic behavior were defined as follows. At level 1, the helper did not eXpress that he was listening, understanding, or being sensitive to the feelings of the client, the helper 94 responses detracted significantly from the communications of the client. At level 2, the counselor tended to re- spond in such a way that he subtracted noticeable affect from the client's communication. At level 3, the helper responded to the client with affect and meaning that was essentially the same as that expressed by the client, but the counselor responded to the client's surface feelings only. This was defined as the minimal level of facili- tative interpersonal functioning. At level 4, the helper's responses added deeper feeling and meaning to the client's eXpressions. At level 5, the counselor responded with full awareness of the client's deepest thoughts and feel- ings and accurately expressed the client's feelings at a level deeper than the client himself was able to eXpress (this scale is further described in Appendix A). The E scale received extensive validation in different counseling settings and with a variety of client populations (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967). The scale was re- written to attempt to increase greater reliability. Martin, Carkhuff, and Berenson (1966) reported the following rate—rerate reliabilities for three judges over a one-week interval--.99, .99, and .95. Inter- rater reliabilities in the same study were .93, .87, and .73. Carkhuff, Krathochvil, and Friel (1968) reported intrarater reliabilities of .90, .99, and .94 for this scale. Interrater reliabilities in this study were .88, .87, and .85. 95 The E scale was used in this investigation to measure the level of helper interview behavior in all treatment and comparison groups. The Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) This scale was developed by Kagan and his associ- ates (1967) to measure four dimensions of a counselor's response to a client communication. The CVRS required a rater to describe the counselor's response in terms of four dimensions. These dimensions were affective— cognitive, understanding-nonunderstanding, specific- nonspecific, and exploratory-nonexploratory. An affective response made reference to or encouraged some affective or feeling aspect of the client's communication while a cognitive response referred primarily to the cognitive component of a client's statement. Counselor understand- ing was defined as the helper's ability to convey to the client his awareness of and sensitivity to the client's feelings. The counselor responded in a nonunderstanding manner when he failed to be aware of and sensitive to the client's statements. A specific counselor response dealt with the core of the client's concern; a nonspecific response either dealt peripherally or with complete disregard for the client's concern. An exploratory response encouraged the client to explore his feelings and provided him with an opportunity to explore his concerns whereas a nonexploratory response typically 96 restricted the client's freedom to explore (these dimensions are more fully described in Appendix B). The CVRS was used to rate a series of individual client-counselor verbal units. The unit was defined as a client statement and a counselor response. The usual procedure was to rate 20 consecutive client-counselor units in an interview. The judge was required to describe every counselor response on each of the four dimensions. The CVRS had been used to measure the change in counselor behavior within and between various approaches to training. Kagan and his associates (1967) described three studies designed to determine interrater reliability on the CVRS. An overview of these studies is presented in Table 3.4. The reliability coefficients for these samples are presented in Table 3.5. Interrater relia- bility was calculated using Ebel's technique (Ebel in Mehrens & Ebel, 1967). Table 3.4 Overview of Three Studies Designed to Determine Interrater Reliability on the CVRS Stud No. of No. of Mode of Type of y Judges Tapes Presentation Rating A 4 6 Audio 20 responses each tape B 3 14 Video 20 responses each tape C 3 53 Video 4-minute timed segment 97 Table 3.5 Average Tape and Item Interrater Reliability Coefficients for Each Dimension of the CVRS on Three Separate Studiesa Aff.- Und.- Spec.- Exp.- Study Cogn. Nund. Nspec. Nexp. A Tape .80 .81 .70 .87 Item .638 .568 .524 .549 B Tape .824 .873 . .682 .839 Item_ .625 .506 .454 .450 C Tapeb Item .637 .508 .524 .549 aThe formula used to derive the estimate of the reliability of individual ratings is: M- - M x M— + (k-l)M x r: where 3 ll mean square for error, M; = mean square for persons, and k = number of raters. bBecause of the unequal number of rateable re- sponse, units within any given timed segment, only item reliability estimates were calculated. Aff.-Cogn. = Affective—cognitive; Und.-Nund. = Understanding-nonunderstanding; Spec.-Nspec. = Specific- nonspecific; Exp.—Nexp. = Exploratory-nonexploratory. 98 Spivack (1970) and Grzegorek (1971) also reported interrater reliabilities of two judges using the CVRS. In these studies, the CVRS was used to measure change in counselor behavior pre-training, mid-training, post- training, and delayed post-training. The interrater reliabilities in these studies are presented in Table 3.6. Table 3.6 Interrater Reliabilities—-CVRS Experimental Study Condition A U S E Total Spivack Role-Play Interview (1970) Pre-Measure .972 .977 .977 .985 Mid-Measure .962 .979 .981 .968 Post-Measure .992 .997 .971 .992 Coached Client Interview Pre-Measure .940 .939 .939 .935 Mid-Measure .979 .971 .974 .979 Post-Measure .950 .943 .943 .930 Grzegorek Interview Tapes (1971) Pre-Tapes .93 .96 .96 .96 .96 Post-Tapes .99 .98 .98 .98 .99 Delayed Post- Tapes .99 .97 .98 .98 .98 The data from the Kagan studies A and C were used to further test the validity of the scale's dimensions (Kagan, pp pp., 1967). This was done to see if the scale differentiated counselors at different levels of experi— ence. The data are presented in Table 3.7. In all but 99 one case there was a clear differentiation between counse- lors at different ability levels on the CVRS dimensions. Table 3.7 Mean Scores of Six Counselors at Various Levels of Training on Each of the Dimensions of the CVRS Counselor Affect Und. Spec. Exp. Eff. Total A 9.75 17.25 15.25 11.25 15.75 69.25 B 9.25 9.00 7.75 10.00 7.50 43.50 C 11.25 17.25 12.00 16.25 15.50 72.25 D 2.25 7.50 5.25 8.00 3.50 26.60 E 5.25 6.75 6.00 8.00 5.75 31.75 F 1.00 2.00 1.00 5.25 .50 9.75 Counselor: A = Ph.D. candidate rated good by supervisor; B = Ph.D. candidate rated weak by supervisor; C = School counselor rated good by supervisor; D = M.A. practicum rated poor by supervisor; E = Beginning practicum; F = Beginning practicum. These reported reliability and validity studies (Kagan, pp pp., 1967; Spivack, 1970; Grzegorek, 1971) indi- cated that the CVRS could be used to rate helper interview behavior. In this investigation, the CVRS was used to rate helper interview behavior across all treatment and comparison groups. Collection of Data Two weeks after training, two samples of trainee helping behavior were taken. The Affective Sensitivity 100 Scale (ASS) was administered to both the eXperimental treatment groups (ITM-I, ITM-II) and to the comparison groups (ETM, PCG). This instrument attempted to measure the levels of helper affective sensitivity across all groups following training. At the same time, an audio— taped interview with a student (selected by the trainee) was collected from all subjects in the experimental and comparison groups. This interview was to be a sample of the level of helper behavior in an interview. These audio— tapes were later rated by judges using the CVRS and E scales. Eight weeks after training, the Affective Sensi- tivity Scale (ASS) was again administered to the experi— mental groups (ITM-I, ITM-II). This delayed post-test was administered to measure the level of trainee affective sensitivity two months after the training period. At the same time, an audiotaped interview with a student was collected from each subject in the experimental groups (ITM-I and ITM-II) and from each member of the professional counselor group. This interview was to be a sample of the level of helper behavior in an interview. These audio- tapes were later rated by judges using the CVRS and E scales. These post and delayed post criterion measures were used to investigate the levels of helper behavior in all groups. The scores on these measures were analyzed 101 to test the differences between the eXperimental and comparison groups and within these groups over a period of time. Preparation of the Audiotapes for Rating All audiotapes, post and delayed post, from all groups-~experimental and comparison—-were randomly assigned to three master rating tape groups. This precaution was taken to prevent a rater bias. That is, the identity of the tape could be concealed. The judges could not know whether each audiotape was a post or delayed post measure or whether it represented the experimental groups or the comparison groups. The master rating tapes were prepared in the following manner. A three-minute excerpt from the first third of each interview was rerecorded onto the master tape. Following a one-minute pause, a five-minute portion from the middle of the interview was rerecorded. Follow- ing another minute pause, a three-minute segment from the final portion of the interview was rerecorded. This procedure was followed for all tapes. ‘When all tapes had been transcribed to the master tapes, the three master tape rating groups were then randomly assigned to judges. The judges independently rated helper behavior on these master tapes using the CVRS dimensions and the E scale. Totals for each judge 102 on the CVRS dimensions were calculated. Totals for each judge on the E scale were computed and then averaged. Reliabilipy of Judges' Ratings Reliability of the three judges' ratings was established on a sample of 12 interview tapes. In this study, the judges independently rated counselor responses on the four dimensions of the CVRS scale. The totals for each dimension were calculated. In addition, these judges independently assigned a rating on the E scale for three portions of each interview. The three ratings then were totaled and averaged. Hoyt (in Mehrens & Ebel, 1967) suggested a formula for estimating the reliability of a test which was based on analysis of variance. Hoyt proposed that this model provided a better estimate of test reliability than the split-half method. Ebel (in Mehrens & Ebel, 1967) applied Hoyt's proposal to two ways of estimating rater reliability. One procedure estimated the reliability of judges over a set of ratings. This technique was used when several judges' ratings were available for each of a number of subjects. The second approach estimated the reliability of individual ratings. This technique estimated the reliability when only an individual judge's score was available for a number of subjects. This second procedure was therefore appropriate for this study. The data for 103 this investigation were based on an individual judge's score. The reliability formula suggested by Ebel was as follows: M— — M r = X M— + (k-1)M x where r = the reliability coefficient, k = the number of raters, M = the Mean Square of tapes by rater interaction, and M; = the Mean Square of tapes. The reliability raw data for the CVRS and E scales are presented in Appendix H. Table 3.8 presents the Analysis of Variance Table and computation of the raters' relia- bility on the CVRS. Table 3.9 presents the Analysis of Variance Table and computation of the raters' reliability on the E scale. Table 3.8 indicated that the reliability for the ratings on the CVRS was .747. Table 3.9 indicated that the reliability for the ratings on the E scale was .479. While the reliability coefficient for the CVRS was acceptable, the reliability coefficient for the E scale was low. 104 Table 3.8 ANOVA Table and Computation of Reliability of Ratings on the CVRS (Counselor Verbal Response Scale) Sources Reduced Sum Degrees of Mean of Squares Freedom Square Grand Mean 41480.111 1 41480.111 Total 7607.889 35 217.368 Rater 1919.389 2 959.694 Tape 4729.222 11 429.929 Rater x Tape 959.278 22 43.604 r = 429.92923 - 43.602525 Table 3.9 429.92923 + (3-l)43.603535 = ANOVA Table and Computation of Reliability of Ratings on the E Scale Source Reduced Sum Degrees of Mean S of Squares Freedom Square Grand Mean 131.637 1 131.637 Total 7.811 35 0.225 Rater 1.435 2 0.717 Tape 4.208 11 0.383 Rater x Tape 2.238 22 0.102 r .382572 - .101716 .479 .382572 + (3-1).1017l6 = 105 Analypis of the Data The unit for statistical analysis in this study was defined as a group mean. Group means must be used since treatments were administered independently to groups of trainees. Individual scores were not appropriate for statistical analysis (Campbell & Stanley, 1963). Group means were calculated for the ASS scores; for the A, E, U, and S subscores of the CVRS; for the composite CVRS scores; and for the EU average ratings. Two separate analyses were needed in this investi- gation. A multivariate analysis of variance tested the differences in means between the four treatment groups on the two-week posttest. A repeated measures analysis of variance tested the differences in means between the three treatment groups and between the post and delayed-post tests. The Multivariate Analysis of Variance Since it was necessary to evaluate six dependent variables--the ASS, A, U, E, S subscales of the CVRS, and the EU--simultaneously, a multivariate analysis of vari— ance procedure was used to analyze the posttest data of the four treatment groups—-ITM-I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG. Thus, this analysis included an overall test of the significance of the main effects of treatments and measures between the four treatment groups. Since this 106 procedure is insensitive to differences in the scaling of variables, the raw data could be used for analysis. Scheffé post-hoc comparisons were used to test the differ— ences between the means of specific treatment groups if separate univariate tests were significant. The assumptions underlying this model included the following. The dependent variables were assumed to be multivariate normal in distribution. The variances of the dependent variables were assumed to be equal across treatment groups. Errors of measurement were assumed to be normally distributed across the treatment populations (Cooley & Lohnes, 1971; Morrison, 1967). Hummel and Sligo (1971) suggested that the multi- variate model could be especially useful when the cost of a Type I error-—false rejection of the hypotheses—-was high. Rppeated Measures Analysis of Variance A repeated measures analysis of variance was used to analyze the general effects of the ITM-I, ITM-II, and PCG treatments across all the dependent variables at both the post and delayed-post test. In this analysis, the dependent variables were treated as levels of helper be- havior. Scores on each of these variables were converted to a standard metric by dividing the raw score on each variable by the standard deviation of the pooled variance across times and groups for that variable. Scores were 107 transformed to obtain additivity of treatment effects accross measures. The ANOVA assumption of independence was met since the group means were used as the experi— mental unit and each experimental unit was independent of every other one. The Geisser-Greenhouse conservative F test was used in this analysis since there was some question concerning the assumption of equal population correlation coefficients. The assumption of equal pOpu— lation correlation coefficients might have been violated in this investigation. That is, the correlation between the CVRS and the EU in this sample might not be equal to either the correlation between the CVRS and the ASS or to the correlation between the EU and ASS. Thus, this analysis tested for the main effects of treatments, times, or periods of testing, and repeated measures. This analysis also tested for the effects of the treatment-measure interaction, time-measure inter— action, and treatment-time—measure interaction. Tukey post-hoc comparisons were used to test the differences between treatments. Hypotheses The hypotheses for the between groups, treatments, and measures analyses were as follows: Hol: There will be no differences in the means of the four training models--ITM-I, ITM—II, ETM, and PCG--or the ASS, CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, CVRS-S, and EU variables. Ho : Ho HO HO H0 108 There will be no differences in the means of any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and ITM—II groups. There will be no differences in the means of any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and ETM groups. There will be no differences in the means of any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and PCG groups. There will be no differences in the means of any of the dependent variables between the ITM—II and ETM groups. There will be no differences in the means of any of the dependent variables between the ITM-II and PCG groups. The hypotheses for the within treatments, times, and measures analyses were as follows. H07: There will be no differences in the means of the treatment groups (ITM-I, ITM-II, PCG) on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables, collec- tively or individually, between the post and delayed-post testing periods. There will be no interaction of treatments, measures, and testing periods. There will be no interaction of treatment and administration periods. Summary Thirty-five undergraduate "helpers" were randomly assigned to one of two experimental training procedures. One approach incorporated rating scales, role-playing, IPR 109 videotape feedback and recall, and client interviews for training of paraprofessional helpers. The second approach used rating scales, role-playing, group feedback and dis— cussion, and client interviews for training of lay helpers. Both the levels of affective sensitivity of the experimental groups and five measures of their interview behavior were compared with the levels of these behaviors evidenced in two comparison groups. The treatment comparison group con- sisted of 22 resident assistants trained extensively in the IPR process over a period of six months. A second comparison group included nine professional counselors. The overall design used in this study incorporated two schemes. A posttest design was used to test the effec- tiveness of training on helper behavior between the four treatment groups. A post-delayed posttest design tested the effectiveness of training within the two experimental treatment groups and one comparison group over a period of two months. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF THE RESULTS An analysis of the data is presented in this chapter. Each hypothesis is restated. The data, analyses, and results relevant to each hypothesis are discussed. Hypotheses Related to the Between Treatment Groups Design Ho - There will be no differences in the means of the four training models--ITM—I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG--on the ASS, CVRS-A, CVRS—U, CVRS-E, CVRS-S, and EU variables. This hypothesis was formulated to compare group means between the four groups used in this study-—the intensive modified IPR training, the intensive modified communication skills training, the extensive IPR training, and the professional counselors group. A multivariate analysis of variance technique was used to test this hypothesis. Since this statistical procedure is insensi- tive to differences in the scaling of variables, the raw data were analyzed. The overall alpha level was set at .10 to compensate for the risk of a Type II error--false acceptance of the hypothesis. Since this investigator 110 111 wanted to increase the probability that the test would correctly reject the null hypothesis, the alpha level was set at .10 rather than at .05 or .01. The raw data-- group and cell means--for the four treatment groups are presented in Table 4.1. The multivariate F test statistic for testing the equality of mean vectors was 2.0059 with 18 and 14.6274 degrees of freedom. This test was significant at the .10 level, indicating that the treatments differed in the multivariate case. A series of univariate F tests was run to test the differences between treatment levels on each of the six variables-~ASS, CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, CVRS-S, and EU. This procedure required that the sum of the alpha levels in the six univariate F tests not exceed the overall alpha level. Therefore, the alpha level set for each of the univariate F tests was .0167. Results of the univariate F tests are presented in Table 4.2. Results of the univariate tests suggested that on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS—E, and EU treatments differed. Differences were not significant on the ASS and CVRS-S variables. Since there were significant differences on the CVRS-A, CVRSéU,3CVRS-E, and EU means between the four treatment groups, Scheffé post-hoc comparisons were used to identify which of the comparison groups differed. The following hypotheses described the comparisons of interest. 112 .mcmmz Hamu u x “mmsomo n w “bcmEummHB u B "@600 osm.m mms.ma omm.ms mmm.ms mmo.m omm.ms m mm.s oo.m mm.m mm.HH oa.m om.ms saw 166ml oo.m om.ss oo.mH om.ms oo.k oo.~s mam a os.m om.oa 00.04 oo.ms mm.s om.sv «Ho 9 mm.m ms.ms ms.ma om.os o~.m oo.mv Haw ome.m www.ms om~.ss mem.ma mmm.m Nsm.ss m sv.m mm.ms mm.ss mm.ma 4H.m om.Hs oau 129ml ms.~ mm.ms ms.HH ms.qa om.os oo.ms mm m Gm.m sa.ma 05.4H 4H.ss mm.k mm.ms me a mm.~ on.ms H4.ma No.ms Ha.m mm.ms so ome.s so~.m oop.m mam.m mmo.a www.mm m . . . . . . HHuzeHc as H mm NH 00 m 00 OH so so am mu m om.a os.s os.s ov.o ov.m oo.mm me e ms.H ss.s om.s mm.m mm.H mm.om so has.a mmv.oa mmm.k 504.0H osm.~ kmo.mm m mm.a oo.ma om.oa om.ma om.m no.4m mu AHIZBMV so.s om.m o~.m ov.m oe.m km.km mm a 4G.H om.m oo.m om.m mm.a mm.He Ho am mumm>o summ>o oumm>o dumm>o mm< masons mam>mq mmHsmmmz “cmfimmHB <>oz< mumaum>fluasz one MOM names Hamu paw msouollmpmo 3mm H.e magma 113 Table 4.2 Univariate F Test Results ASS 10.938961 5.4845 CVRS-A 4.097002 7.7152* CVRS-U 5.973748 8.5660* CVRS-E 8.922307 9.7458* CVRS-S 10.095882 2.1510 EU 0.168827 7.2492* *Significant at the .10 level. Ho 2: HO HO HO HO There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and ITM—II groups. There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM—I and ETM groups. There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM—I and PCG groups. There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM—II and ETM groups. There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM-II and PCG groups. The Scheffé method of post-hoc comparisons per- Initted all pairwise and complex comparisons between 114 treatment levels in an analysis which had unequal cell frequencies. In this study Tl had an N of 3, T2 had an N of 3, T had an N of 4, and T had an N of 4. Thus the 3 4 Scheffé method was appropriate for the comparisons of interest. Since the univariate F tests failed to reject the hypotheses on the ASS and CVRS-S measures, no com— parisons were warranted. As for the univariate ANOVA's, an alpha level of .0167 was required for the post-hoc analyses in order to maintain an overall alpha level of .10. Results of the Scheffé comparisons on the CVRS-A are presented in Table 4.3 Table 4.3 Differences in the Group Means on the CVRS-A Treatment T1 T2 T3 T4 Levels (ITM—I) (ITM-II) (ETM) (PCG) Treatment Means 2.910 1.633 8.293 6.053 T1 (ITM—I) 2.910 -l.277 -5.383 -3.l43 T2 (ITM-II) 1.633 -6.660* -4.420 T3 (ETM) 8.293 2.240 *Significant Scheffé pairwise comparisons (a < .10). 115 Using the Scheffé formula, the absolute difference in the means of the pairwise comparisons--ITM-I versus ETM, ITM-II versus ETM, ITM-I versus PCG, and ITM-II versus PCG--on the CVRS-A had to exceed a value of 6.324 in order to be significant at the parison ITM-I versus ITM—II, the difference had to be .10 or less level. For the com- 6.744. Thus, on the CVRS-A, the Scheffé post-hoc compari- sons indicated that there was a significant difference between ITM-II and ETM groups. Significant differences were not detected for the remaining pairwise comparisons on the CVRS-A. Results of the Scheffé post-hoc comparisons on the CVRS—U are presented in Table 4.4. Table 4.4 Differences in the Group Means on the CVRS-U Treatment T1 T2 T3 T4 Levels (ITM-I) (ITM-II) (ETM) (PCG) Treatment Means 10.467 8.243 15.273 16.383 T1 (ITM-I) 10.467 2.224 —4.806 -5.916 T2 (ITM—II) 8.243 -7.030 -8.l40* T3 (ETM) 15.273 -1.110 *Significant Scheffé pairwise comparison (a i .10). 116 Using the Scheffé formula, the absolute differ— ences in the means of the pairwise comparisons--ITM-I versus ETM, ITM-II versus ETM, ITM-I versus PCG, and ITM—II versus PCG--on the CVRS-U had to exceed a value of 7.613 in order to be significant at the .10 or less level. For the comparison ITM-I versus ITM—II, the difference had to be 8.144. Thus, on the CVRS-U, the Scheffé post-hoc comparison indicated that there was a significant differ- ence between ITM-II and PCG groups. Significant differ- ences were not detected for the remaining pairwise com- parisons on the CVRS-U. Results of the Scheffé post-hoc pairwise comparisons on the CVRS-E are presented in Table 4.5. Table 4.5 Differences in the Group Means on the CVRS-E Treatment T1 T2 T3 T4 Levels (ITM—I) (ITM-II) (ETM) (PCG) Treatment Means 7.233 5.700 —4.047 15.820 T1 (ITM—I) 7.233 1.533 -7.047 -8.587 T2 (ITM-II) 5.700 -8.580 -10.120* T3 (ETM) 14.280 -1.54 *Significant Scheffé pairwise comparison (a i .10). 117 Using the Scheffé formula, the absolute differ- ence in the means of the pairwise comparisons--ITM-I versus ETM, ITM-II versus ETM, ITM-I versus PCG, and ITM-II versus PCG-—on the CVRS-E had to exceed a value of 9.306 in order to be significant at the .10 level. For the comparison ITM-I versus ITM-II, the difference had to be 9.951. Thus, on the CVRS-E, the Scheffé post-hoc comparisons indicated that there was a significant differ— ence between the ITM-II and PCG groups. Significant differences were not detected for the remaining pairwise comparisons on the CVRS-E. Results of the Scheffé post-hoe comparisons on the EU are presented in Table 4.6. Table 4.6 Differences in the Group Means on the EU Treatment T1 T2 T3 T4 Levels (ITM-I) (ITM—II) (ETM) (PCG) Treatment Means 1.767 1.640 2.630 2.810 T1 (ITM-I) 1.767 0.137 -.863 -1.043 T2 (ITM-II) 1.630 -l.000 -1.180 T3 (ETM) 2.630 -0.l80 118 Using the Scheffé formula, the absolute difference in the means of the pairwise comparisons-—ITM—I versus ETM, ITM-II versus ETM, ITM-I versus PCG, and ITM-II versus PCG-- on the EU had to exceed a value of 1.280 in order to be significant at the .10 level. For the comparison ITM-I versus ITM-II, the difference had to be 1.367. On the EU, significant differences were not detected for any of the pairwise comparisons. The post-hoc analyses of the between treatment groups differences failed to reject Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4. Hypothesis 5 was rejected only in the case of the CVRS-A variable. Hypothesis 6 was rejected for the CVRS-U and CVRS-E variables. These differences were not pre- dicted in Chapter I. Earlier predictions hypothesized that there would be no differences between the treatment groups. Failure to reject Hypotheses 2, 3, and 4 may have been due to the small sample sizes or to large variances in the variables of interest in this study. Hypotheses of the Within Treatment Gropps Design Ho ‘ There will be no differences in the means of the treatment groups (ITM-I, ITM—II, PCG) on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables, collec- tively or individually, between the post and delayed-post testing periods. Ho ° There will be no interactions between treat- ments, measures, and testing periods. 119 Ho : There will be no interactions between treat- ment and test administration periods. These hypotheses were formulated to compare the group means on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables within the three comparison groups—-ITM-I, ITM—II, and PCG——across the post and delayed-post testing periods. A repeated measures analysis of variance was used to test the differ- ences in the means of the ITM-I, ITM-II, and PCG groups on all the dependent variables, across the post and delayed- post testing periods. Since the repeated measures tech- niques were sensitive to different scaling metrics, the raw scores--group means--on each of the variables had to be converted to a standard metric. These transformations were performed by dividing each of the raw scores on each of the variables by the standard deviation of the pooled variance across times and groups for that variable. The scores were transformed to obtain additivity of effects across measures. Thus the transformed scores rather than the raw scores were used in the analysis. The raw scores are presented in Table 4.7; the transformed group and cell means are presented in Table 4.8. In a repeated measures analysis of variance, the F statistic for testing effects involving a repeated measures dimension assumes that the population corre- lation coefficients between the measures used in this study are equal. The assumption of equal population 120 d d m .Mmm.o u om ecu M0 m “Hom.m u mm>o on» M0 m “om.m u mmd ecu Mo m “mBOHHOM mm OMOB OHQMMMm> some MOM mddOMm cam mmEMu mmOMom mam>ma McmEMmmMM ecu cMcMM3 mmOcMMMm> UOHOOQ ecu MO chMMmM>mp cMmcsmum mce “muoz .cmwz QSOMO n m “mQOOMO n O “McmEummMe u E ”mpou mmm.m omm.am mo.me mev.m mm.om mo.ov m >H.m mm.mm no.mv ha.m mm.mv em.mv mo oo.m mm.mm wo.mv mm.m Ho.mm no.me mo mom Mw.m mm.mm no.mv mm.m mm.mm hm.mv no omm.a omn.am nmo.nm omm.H www.mm www.mm m mo.m he.mm mk.mm MG.M oo.Mm oo.mm so AHHIEMWV hm.a oo.ma mm.nm om.H oo.am oo.mm mo 9 mm.a mv.mm oo.nm mn.H mm.ma om.om em ooh.a mmm.mm omm.mm nmh.a mvo.Hm nmo.mm m mo.~ oo.sq 5M.vm mm.M om.ov no.4m mo AHuzewc hm.a m~.~m oo.me nw.H om.mm hm.nm No B mm.H Hm.ma hm.mm vo.a mm.mm mm.Hv Ho Dm mm>o mmd Dm mm>o mmfl deOMo mam>mq Aumomucmwmamoc Aumomc MsmEMmmMa m coMMMMMmMcMEpc H :oMMMMMmMcMEc< mmefla mmOMom mQSOMw cOmMMmeOO OOMSB MOM memo 3mm h.v manna .cmmz QSOMO u x “mQDOMw n w “McmEummMB n B "mcou 121 ms4.m msm.m mmm.sM 44m.m sm4.m s4o.mM cums HHMMO>O omm.s 004.m Mmm.om omm.o mmm.m smm.om m Mmo.m 4k~.4 so~.om NmM.4 Mmm.4 no~.o~ mo mmo.m M4o.4 som.om mm4.4 mm4.m som.o~ mo mom mm4.> Mmm.m omo.om Nmo.m 44o.m om4.om so mM4.4 on~.m mmm.44 m44.4 km4.~ m4m.mM m mmk.m 4m4.~ M4~.4M 44m.4 N4~.m 4mm.hs mu AHHuzch mam.m mmm.M st.4M mn~.4 omM.~ omm.sM 46 a 444.4 ons.m Mem.44 Mno.m Mmo.~ mom.44 mo hMo.m moo.m kmm.4M mmo.m omm.m 044.4M m mmm.m mam.4 mMM.mM mwm.m mm~.m 04m.mM mo iMuzemc sma.4 kmm.m 4mm.mM sms.4 mam.~ www.ma No a mmm.4 mmm.M OHM.MM Nsm.4 mom.m mom.mH Mo am mm>o mm4 am mm>o mm< mQDOMm mam>mq AMmOMIpowmaoov humomc MamaummMa m GOMMMMMmMcMEp< H GOMMMMMmMcMEpd mmMoom cmEMoMmQMMB m.v OHQMB 122 correlation coefficients might have been violated in this investigation. That is, the correlation between the CVRS and the EU in this sample may not be equal to either the correlation between the CVRS and the ASS or to the corre- lation between the EU and ASS. Since there was some doubt concerning the assumption of equal population correlation coefficients, the Geisser-Greenhouse conservative F test-- which does not make the assumption-~was used in the analy- sis of the data. This investigation predicted in Chapter I that the levels of helper behavior would remain at the same levels over a period of time. Thus Hypothesis 7 predicted no differences on the A (times) main effect and on the AM interaction (times by measures) within the three compari- son groups. In the test of the A main effect (post and delayed-post periods), the critical value of the conser- vative F test, at the .05 alpha level of significance was 5.99. This new alpha level was set to control for the possibility of a Type I error—-a false rejection Of the null hypothesis. The critical value of the conservative F test for the AM interaction was also 5.99 at the .05 level. As predicted the tests of the A main effect and of the AM interaction failed to reject Hypothesis 7. (See ANOVA table of transformed scores presented in Table 4.9.) Caution must be observed in interpreting the failure to reject a null hypothesis. Failure to reject a null hypothesis does not mean that the hypothesis is 123 Table 4.9 ANOVA Table of Transformed Scores Sources of Sum of Degrees of Mean F Variation Squares Freedom Square Ratio T 98.823 2 49.411 54.569* G(T) 5.433 6 .905 A .028 l .028 .028 TA .432 2 .216 .219 GA(T) 5.922 6 .987 M 2173.410 2 1086.705 700.089* TM 3.885 4 .971 .626 GM(T) 18.627 12 1.552 AM .207 2 .103 .202 TAM .229 4 .057 .112 GAM(T) 6.126 12 .510 Total 2313.120 53 *Significant at a i .05 using a conservative test. Code: trations; M T = Treatment; G = Groups; A = Adminis- Measures; Main Effects TAM; Interactions = TA, TM, AM, TAM; Error Terms = G(T), GA(T), GM(T), GAM(T). 124 accepted. Statistical inference is not good in supporting a null hypothesis because there is an unknown probability of a Type II error-—false acceptance of a hypothesis. Examination of the raw data in Table 4.7 will reveal that there are small differences in observed raw data metric. For instance, the EU scale metric itself ranges from one to five. The range of scores on this scale for the treat— ment means in this study from post to delayed-post tests is as follows: T1 = 1.767 versus 1.760, T = 1.630 versus 2 1.620, and T3 = 2.443 versus 2.593. Likewise, the range of scores--treatment means--on the ASS is as follows: Tl 38.057 versus 38.280, T2 = 38.287 versus 37.027, and T3 = 46.03 versus 46.03. This seems to be a quite narrow range of scores which might occur in helper behaviors in a larger pOpulation. Hypothesis 8 predicted no differences on the TAM interaction (treatment by times by measures). In the test of the TAM interaction, the critical value of the conser- vative F test at the .05 level of significance was 5.14. The test of the TAM interaction failed to permit rejection of Hypothesis 8. Hypothesis 9 predicted no differences on the TA interaction (treatment by times). In the test of the TA interaction, the critical value of the conservative F test at the .05 level of significance was 5.14. The test of the TA interaction failed to permit rejection of Hypothesis 9. 125 Failure to reject Hypotheses 7, 8, and 9 seemed to indicate that there were no differences within the compari- son group means as measured by the ASS, CVRS, and EU vari- ables across the post and delayed—post testing periods. Inspection of the ANOVA table did reveal significant differences (p p .05) between the means of the comparison groups as tested by the T (treatment) and by the M (mea- sures) main effects. This investigation was not inter— ested in a measures main effect. But this investigation was interested in treatment main effects. The means of the treatment main effects were: T = 8.356, T = 7.890, 1 2 and T = 10.964. Since the test of the treatment main 3 effect was significant at the .05 level, Tukey post-hoc comparisons were used to investigate the T main effect. The absolute differences in the means of these comparisons had to exceed 2.383 in order to be significant at the .05 level. Results on the Tukey post-hoc comparisons are presented in Table 4.10. Table 4.10 Post-Hoc Comparisons of the T Main Effect Treatment T1 T2 T3 Levels (ITM-I) (ITM-II) (PCG) Treatment Means 8.357 7.890 10.964 Tl (ITM—I) .467 -2.607* T2 (ITM-II) -3.074* *Significant Tukey comparison (a i .05). 126 Results of the Tukey comparisons indicated that there were significant differences (a i .05) in the treat- ment means for the following pairs-~ITM-I versus PCG and ITM-II versus PCG. Thus it seemed that in this analysis there were significant differences between both of the experimental groups (ITM-I and ITM-II) and the professional counselor group (PCG) as measured by the treatment means-- a composite of all the variables for that treatment level. These results are somewhat contradictory to the findings of the multivariate test. The multivariate analysis re- vealed no statistical differences between ITM-I and the PCG groups. Differences between the ITM-II and PCG groups were found on the CVRS-U and CVRS-E. The repeated measures findings seem much clearer than the multivariate findings. One reason for the discrepancy in the findings may be that the repeated measures analysis is a more sensitive and powerful test than the multivariate tests for the type of multivariate data in this study. The lack of a TM and TA interaction suggests that the pairwise differences between treatment levels holds across all three independent vari- ables. Summary A multivariate analysis of variance was used to test the overall difference in group means between the two experimental training paradigms for undergraduate paraprofessional helpers (ITM-I, ITM-II) and two 127 comparison groups of helpers (ETM, PCG). The null hypothesis that there would be no differences between treatment levels was rejected in the multivariate case (a i .10). In the univariate case, the treatment group means on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables were shown to be significantly different (a i .10). Scheffé post-hoc pariwise comparisons were used to test the differences between the four treatment levels (ITM-I, ITM-II, ETM, PCG) on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables. Significant differences were found between the ITM-II and ETM groups on the CVRS-A and between the ITM-II and PCG groups on the CVRS-U and CVRS-E variables. Significant differences were not detected between the ITM-I and ITM-II groups, between the ITM—I and ETM groups, or between the ITM-I and PCG groups on any of the six dependent variables when analyzed by the multivariate. Significant differences were also not detected between the ITM—II and PCG groups on the CVRS-A and EU variables and significant differences were not detected between the ITM—II and ETM groups on the CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU vari- ables. These results are partly contradicted in the later analyses. A repeated measures analysis of variance was used to test the differences within the group means of the two experimental training groups (ITM-I, ITM-II) and one comparison group (PCG). The null hypothesis--that there would be no difference within the means of the three groups 128 on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables--collective1y or individually--across the testing periods, failed to be rejected. The hypotheses testing the TAM and TA inter- actions also failed to be rejected. But the treatment main effect was significant at the .05 level. Significant differences between both the experimental groups (ITM-I, ITM-II) and the professional counselors group were found. Table 4.11 presents a summary of the hypotheses of interest in this study and the results of the analyses. The summary and conclusions of this investigation are discussed in Chapter V. 129 Table 4.11 Summary of the Hypotheses and Results Hypothesis Test a Decision 1 Multivariate ANOVA .10 Reject 2 Scheffé post—hoc comparisons .0167 Failed to reject 3 Scheffé post—hoc comparisons .0167 Failed to reject 4 Scheffé post-hoc comparisons .0167 Failed to reject 5 Scheffé post—hoc Reject for the comparisons .0167 CVRS-A 6 Scheffé post-hoc Reject for the comparisons .0167 CVRS-U and CVRS-E 7 Repeated Measures ANOVA .05 Failed to reject 8 Repeated Measures ANOVA .05 Failed to reject 9 Repeated Measures ANOVA .05 Failed to reject CHAPTER V SUMMARY, DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS Summary This study compared the effectiveness of two different approaches to the training of undergraduate para- professional mental health workers or "helpers." The in- vestigation attempted to determine the kinds of trainee behavior evidenced following an intensive 40-hour training program conducted during five consecutive days. Two train- ing programs were used--a modified IPR experience and a modified communication skills training program. The out- comes of both treatments were compared to each other, to a comparison group of resident assistants trained exten- sively in a 38-hour IPR program which was conducted over a six-month period of time, and to a comparison group of professional counselors. Both the experimental training procedures--the modified IPR experience and the modified communications skills program--were designed to increase the "inter- personal communications skills" of student helpers. Four general goals underlying both of the training programs included the following: to make trainees more aware of 130 131 and sensitive to the feelings and concerns of their peers; to provide trainees with listening and communication skills which would facilitate understanding between them and the students desiring help; to offer student helpers effective ways of responding to and behaving with students; and to give trainees an increased awareness of the impact of the helper-student interaction in the college and residence hall. Therefore the "interpersonal communication skills" of interest in this study included both helper empathic understanding and the helper's ability to use affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory responses to stu- dent concerns. Helper empathic understanding was defined as the helper's sensitivity to current feelings, as evi- denced by his verbal facility to communicate this under- standing to a student in a language attuned to the peer's current feelings (Carkhuff, 1969b). Helper affective responses made reference to the emotions, feelings, or fears of a client. Understanding responses communicated to the student that the helper understood or was trying to understand what the student was expressing, both verbally and non-verbally. Specific responses were the helper's honest statements of the core concerns being presented, either explicitly or implicitly, verbally or non-verbally, by the student. Exploratory responses indicated whether the helper encouraged or permitted the student to explore his concerns. 132 Carkhuff and his associates (1964, 1965a, 1965b) proposed a paradigm for the training of paraprofessionals in helping behaviors. Carkhuff posited that there was a central core of facilitative conditions in all helping processes. Four helper variables in this process were defined as empathic understanding (E), regard (R), genuine- ness (G), and concreteness (C). The client variable was defined as self-exploration (Ex). Carkhuff then devised a five-step training program in the facilitative dimensions for lay helpers. Trainees were didactically taught the therapeutic dimensions of E, R, G, C, and Ex. Trainees then learned to discriminate the levels of each of these dimensions by rating audiotaped helper and client responses using the research scales. During empathy training, trainees wrote their own responses to audiotaped client statements. The trainees then focused on role-playing helper-client interactions and then evaluating their be- haviors using the research scales. Finally, these helpers interviewed actual clients and, following each interview, received feedback about the levels of their facilitative behaviors. Research evidence suggested that a variety of trainee groups could be trained to function at minimally faCilitative levels on the research dimensions. Addi- tional investigations proposed that lay persons could be trained to effect constructive behavior change in their 133 clients. In light of these research studies, it seemed that the Carkhuff training model might be effective in training undergraduate paraprofessional helpers. Yet, the research had not compared the effects of the Carkhuff model with other approaches to training. Kagan and his associates (1967) develOped the Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) as another prOposed training model in helping behaviors. This model was originally developed as an approach to counselor edu- cation. These researchers sought ways of gaining knowl- edge about the underlying dynamics of human relationships; thus, the thoughts and feelings of both the counselor and the client during the interview process were of special interest. Techniques were devised which would allow a helper or a client to interact at one time and then intro- spect concerning that interaction at another time. Affect stimulation, videotape feedback, and stimulated recall were used to facilitate the interpersonal learning process. Thus, the IPR process involved four steps--a lecture on facilitative conditions; stimulated confron- tation recall (filmed stimulus); counselor, client, and mutual recall; and individual interviews. Trainees were didactically taught the elements of effective communication by watching films of experienced counselors with clients and then discussing with a trainer the crucial elements of that interpersonal communication. During stimulated 134 confrontation recall, a client was videotaped while watch- ing stimulus films which portrayed various emotions, such as anger, hostility, and seduction. An inquirer recalled with the client the feelings and thoughts which the client experienced during the original session. During the third phase, the trainees experienced IPR counselor or client recall. Following an interview with a client, an inquirer would review this videotaped interaction with either the counselor or the client. In mutual recall, the last phase of training, the inquirer would recall the videotaped interaction with the counselor and the client together. The four IPR techniques just described were used to facilitate particular kinds of trainee learning. Trainees needed to become aware of and sensitive to the elements of effective communication; they needed to under- stand and become sensitive to the client's communications during the helping process. At the same time, the trainees needed to understand and become sensitive to their own feelings during a counseling session. Finally they needed to become sensitive to the bilateral nature of the counsel— ing interaction; that is, they needed to become sensitized to the reciprocal impact of the counselor on the client and the client on the counselor. Goldberg (1967), Spivack (1970), and Grzegorek (1971) obtained data which supported the notion that the IPR process could be effectively used with counselor trainees. Research also suggested that the 135 Kagan model might be effective in training lay helpers but previous studies have not yet tested this contention. This study compared the relative effectiveness of the Carkhuff and the Kagan training models when used with undergraduate paraprofessional "helpers." In this investi- gation, both training models were modified in the following manner. While Carkhuff usually included the five dimensions of E, R, G, C, and Ex in his five-step training process, this study focused on only two dimensions--the E and Ex scales. Therefore, the modified Carkhuff training model in this study included discrimination training using two rating scales (E and Ex), empathy training, role—playing, group feedback and discussion, and client interviews. The training program was conducted as an intensive 40-hour experience over a period of five days. Kagan's IPR process usually included a lecture on facilitative conditions, stimulated confrontation recall, client or counselor recall, and mutual recall. This study modified the IPR process in the following ways. The CVRS was not used in the lecture on facilitative conditions. The E and Ex scales were used instead. In this way, the CVRS could be used as an unbiased criterion measure across all groups. Stimulation confrontation recall (filmed stimulus) was omitted* and tape-rating and empathy training *This experience was included in the Spivack (1970), (lrzegorek (1971), Schauble (1970), Dendy (1971), and Archer (1971) studies but was not used by Goldberg (1967). 136 were substituted for this phase of training. Thus the intensive modified IPR training model included a lecture on facilitative conditions in the helping process, tape- rating, empathy training, role-playing, videotaped feed- back, client, counselor, and mutual recall, and interview with a client. Thirty—five undergraduate "helpers" were randomly assigned to one of the two experimental training programs --the intensive modified IPR process (ITM-I) or the inten- sive modified communication skills training (ITM-II). Two comparison groups were also used in the analysis of helper behavior. The training comparison group (ETM) in- cluded 22 undergraduate resident assistants who were trained in helping behaviors over a six-month period of time. A second comparison group (PCG) included pro- fessional counselors employed by a university counseling center. Following training, two samples of helper behavior were collected from all groups. An audiotaped interview with a student and a paper-and-pencil test of affective sensitivity were collected two weeks after training in all four treatment groups--ITM-I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG. Eight weeks later, an audiotaped interview with a student and the test of affective sensitivity were again collected in both of the experimental treatment groups—-ITM-I and ITM-II--and in the professional counselor group--PCG. 137 These samples were used to assess the levels of helper functioning in each of the groups used in this study. Six criterion measures were used to evaluate the helper's interview behavior. Carkhuff's Empathic Under- standing Scale (see Appendix A) was used to assess helper empathic responses to student concerns during the audio- taped interview. Kagan's Counselor Verbal Response Scale (the CVRS--see Appendix B) was used to measure the helper's ability to use affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory responses to student communications during the interview process. The composite CVRS score and the CVRS subscores-—CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and CVRS-S--were used in the analyses of helper interview behavior. An addi— tional measure-—Kagan's Affective Sensitivity Scale (the ASS--see Appendix C)--was used as a paper-and-pencil test of the helper's ability to judge what a client, seen in a videotaped counseling session, was feeling about him- self and about his counselor. Thus, the overall design used in this study incor- porated two schemes. A posttest design was used to test the effectiveness of training on helper behavior between the four comparison groups--the intensive modified IPR training, the intensive modified communication skills paradigm, the extensive IPR training experience, and the professional counselors group. A post-delayed post test design tested the effectiveness of training between the 138 two eXperimental treatment groups-~the intensive modified IPR training, the intensive modified communication skills paradigm, and the professional counselors group over a period of two months. Hypotheses relevant to the basic questions in this study were as follows. Hypotheses related to the between treatment groups design Hol: There will be no differences in the means of the four training models--ITM-I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG--on the ASS, CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, CVRS-S, and EU variables. H02: There will be no differences in the means of any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and ITM-II groups. Ho - There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and ETM groups. H04: There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM-I and PCG groups. Ho ' There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM-II and ETM groups. Ho - There will be no differences in the means on any of the dependent variables between the ITM-II and PCG groups. 139 Hypotheses related to the within treatment groups design H07: There will be no differences in the means of the treatment groups--ITM-I, ITM-II, and PCG --on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables, collec- tively or individually, between the post and delayed-post testing periods. H08: There will be no interactions between treatments, measures, and testing periods. H09: There will be no interactions between treatment and test administration periods. A multivariate analysis of variance was used to test the overall differences in group means between the two experimental training paradigms for undergraduate para— professional helpers (ITM-I, ITM-II) and two comparisons (ETM, PCG). The null hypothesis, that there would be no differences between treatment levels on all variables, was rejected in the multivariate case (p < .10). The overall alpha level was set at .10 to compensate for the risk of a Type II error—-false acceptance of the hypothesis. In the univariate case, the means on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables were shown to be significantly different (p < .10) across the four treatment levels. Scheffé post-hoc comparisons were used to test the differ— ences between the four treatment levels--ITM—I, ITM-II, ETM, and PCG--on the CVRS-A, CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU vari— ables. Significant differences (p < .10) were found between the ITM-II and ETM groups on the CVRS-A and between 140 the ITM-II and PCG groups on the CVRS—U and CVRS-E vari- ables. Significant differences were not detected between the ITM-I and ITM-II groups, between the ITM-I and ETM groups, or between the ITM-I and PCG groups on all six of the variables used in this analysis. Significant differ— ences were also not detected between the ITM-II and PCG groups on the CVRS-A and EU variables. Nor were signifi- cant differences detected between the ITM-II and ETM groups on the CVRS-U, CVRS-E, and EU variables. A repeated measures analysis of variance was used to test the differences within and between the group means of the two experimental training groups--ITM-I, ITM-II-— and one comparison group--PCG. The null hypothesis that there would be no difference in the means of the three groups on the ASS, CVRS, and EU variables--collectively or individually--across the testing periods failed to be rejected. The hypothesis testing the TAM and TA inter- actions also failed to be rejected. Though not hypothe- sized in this analysis, a treatment main effect was found (p < .05). Significant differences between both the experimental groups--ITM-I, ITM—II-—and the professional counselors group were evidenced in the repeated measures analysis. Discussion This section will present conclusions drawn from the results of the statistical analyses, will discuss 141 possible limitations of the study and will consider the confounding variables which might have affected the outcome results. Results of the Analysis The comparison groups used in this study included the undergraduates trained intensively in IPR, the helpers trained intensively in a communication skills paradigm, the helpers trained extensively in IPR, and the professional counselors. The multivariate analysis and Scheffé post— hoc comparisons suggested that significant differences in helper behavior existed in the following areas. The pro- fessional counselors used significantly more understanding and exploratory responses to student concerns during an interview than did the undergraduates trained intensively in communication skills. Differences were not detected in interview behavior or levels Of affective sensitivity be— tween the undergraduates trained intensively in IPR and those helpers trained extensively in IPR. There also seemed to be no differences in interview behavior and levels of affective sensitivity between the professional counselors and those helpers trained intensively in IPR. Yet the repeated measures analysis suggested that the pro- fessional counselors functioned at higher levels of empathic understanding, affective sensitivity, and Counse- lor Verbal Response dimensions than either of the eXperi- mental trainee groups--those helpers trained intensively in 142 IPR and those trainees in the communication skills paradigm. The repeated measures results differed with the multi- variate analysis. Traditionally, caution is observed in interpreting the acceptance of null hypothesis. The probability of a Type II error-—false acceptance--cou1d occur in such "acceptance." It is considered safer to assume that statistical inference delineates the conditions of rejecting a hypothesis or of failing to reject a hy- pothesis rather than suggesting the acceptance of null hypothesis. In considering the conclusions drawn from the data in this study, several issues must be considered. These include the levels of helper functioning evidenced follow— ing training, the limited samples of helper functioning in the environment, and the low rater reliability on the EU scale. Levels of Helper Functioning Followinngraining It was interesting to note that even though the trainees in ITM-I and ITM-II were taught the EU scale--the ITM-II group were especially "drilled" on this scale--the interview behaviors of these helpers still remained low. On both the post and delayed-post measures, the level of helper behavior was below level three on the EU scale. Level three was defined by Carkhuff as the minimally facilitative level of helper empathic understanding. This 143 raised some question concerning the use of didactic material to influence helper communicated behavior on a certain dimension, such as the dimension of helper empathic understanding. Cognitive knowledge of a dimension did not seem to guarantee communication of that dimension. In their studies, Carkhuff, Kratochvil, and Friel (1968) sug— gested that the discrimination of the facilitative dimen— sions of empathic understanding, respect, and genuineness did not directly correlate with the communication of those dimensions in an interview process. Carkhuff (1968) claimed that paraprofessional helpers could be trained to function at minimally facili- tative levels of E, R, C, and G in as few as 20 hours of training. The training program in his study was conducted over a period of several weeks. The Dendy (1971) study trained undergraduate paraprofessionals in a 38-hour pro- gram which was conducted over a period of six months. The mean helper behavior evidenced following training in the Dendy (1971) study was 2.630. These studies raised the question whether training conducted over a longer period of time might be more effective than an intensive training experience. Longer periods of training and supervision might effect greater gains in trainee behavior than would the short-term training. Carkhuff (1968) also suggested that lay helpers could function at levels as high (never lower) than 144 professional counselors. Yet, in this study, the repeated measures analysis indicated that the professional counse- lors functioned at higher levels of affective sensitivity, empathic understanding, and CVRS dimensions than did under- graduate helpers in either of the experimental training programs. It seemed that future research was needed to define both the possible effects of long-term training versus short-term training and the efficacy of professional helper functioning on client progress as compared to para- professional functioning in the helper process. The Dendy (1971) study found no significant differ- ences in the levels of helper functioning, as measured by the ASS, CVRS, and EU, between a group of resident assistants trained extensively in the IPR process and a group of professional counselors. The undergraduate paraprofessionals in the Dendy (1971) study had been trained over a period of six months. In contrast, this study found significant differences in the levels of helper functioning between each of the experimental in— tensive trainee groups and a group of professional counselors. Differences in the post-training results between the Dendy study and this study might result from two factors. The total number of training hours were similar in both studies--Dendy used 38 hours, this study used 40 hours. Yet the Dendy training was conducted over a period of six months while this study conducted training 145 over a period of five days. The longer period of training might have allowed trainees to integrate the helper be— haviors more thoroughly than did the intensive training models. Also, this study deleted the stimulation confron- tation (filmed stimulus) recall experience, while the Dendy (1971) study included this technique in training. Future research might be warranted to investigate the effects of this technique on overall trainee growth in helping behaviors. Limited Sample and Range of Helper Behavior Only two kinds of helper behavior were collected across all groups in this study. These samples included helper responses to student concerns in a one interview situation (post and delayed-post times) and a paper-and- pencil test of affective sensitivity (post and delayed- post times). These two samples might not have shown a representative picture of the helpers' behaviors as they occurred in the environment. Several interview samples with different kinds of students might be needed to present a more accurate picture of helper behavior in the interview process. Kagan and his associates (1967) indicated that the CVRS could discriminate the differences in helper behavior in a one interview sample. Goldberg (1967) sug- gested that a four-minute segment was as effective as a 10- or 15—minute segment of interview behavior in 146 determining changes in counselor behavior. Yet examination of the raw data revealed that the range of scores between the treatment groups on the EU scale was quite narrow. The group mean raw scores on the EU scale ranged from 1.630 to 2.810 on the posttest design and from 1.620 to 2.593 on the repeated measures design. Likewise, the range of the composite CVRS transformed scores was small--from 2.270 to 5.400. These ranges of helper behavior seemed to be quite compacted. Low Rater Reliability on the EU Scale The low rater reliability on the EU scale appeared to present another concern in this study. It seemed that the interpretation of the EU results could be questioned. This low reliability could have caused a lack of differen- tiation between the treatment groups on this scale-- especially in the multivariate analysis of variance. Yet, the repeated measures analysis seemed to be a sensitive enough test to be unaffected by the low rater reliability. Differences in treatment levels were detected on the EU scale in the repeated measures analysis. Observations and Speculations During and after the training period, the investi- gator observed several other factors which might have had a bearing on the results of this investigation. On the third day of training, the trainees in both experimental 147 treatments began to experience fatigue. Since the training procedures were collapsed into five consecutive days, trainees seemed to feel many demands on their performance during training. By the fourth and fifth day, these trainees seemed to have reached an "over—saturation" point. That is, since so many new experiences were compacted into such a short time, it might have been difficult for these trainees to integrate the kinds of learning included in the latter days of training. The cumulative effect of such massed practice in both of the intensive training models might have caused a deterioration in trainee learning. Trainees might not have had enough time to integrate the training eXperiences in a period of five days. If the training experiences had been spaced over a period of several weeks, trainees might have had an opportunity to introspect about their helping behaviors, to practice or to experiment with new behaviors in their everyday environ— ment, and to consider ways in which they personally wanted to grow as helpers. In the intensive modified communication skills groups, trainees began to request a sensitivity group experience. During the first two days of training, these "helpers" began to focus on their own and on other trainees' behaviors. While the IPR trainees seemed to be given the Opportunity to introspect about their personal behaviors during IPR recall, the communication skills trainees had less direct Opportunity for introspection. In the ITM-II 148 treatment, group feedback and discussions centered on the use of rating scales to interpret behavior. More direct methods of introspection might have included IPR recall, group counseling, or sensitivity group experience. No training procedures were offered during fall term. This was done to partially control for the confound— ing variable of additional training during the post and delayed—post periods. Yet the investigator received re- quests for additional training from the trainees in both experimental groups. At times, trainees seemed disappointed about having to wait until winter and spring terms for these experiences. Trainees might have found that the treatment procedures started a "growth process" in helper behaviors which later became stifled or frustrated during the waiting period. During winter and spring terms of the same school year, the undergraduate "helpers" asked that the investigator return to their staff meetings to consult with them about their growth as helpers. These eXperiences sug- gested that extended supervision and consultation might be crucial in the training of lay helpers in facilitative communication. Several investigators have defined helper behaviors which seemed to influence client growth. Kagan and his associates (1967) suggested four develOpmental tasks that a helper needed to learn so that he could become effective in his interpersonal communications. The helper needed to 149 become aware of and sensitive to the elements of effective communication. The counselor needed to understand and become sensitive to his own feelings while also attempting to understand and become sensitive to the client's communi- cations during an interview process. Finally, the helper needed to become aware of the bilateral nature of the counseling interaction. Carkhuff and his associates (1964, 1965a, 1965b) posited that a helper needed to learn how to effectively communicate four kinds of helper conditions to the client. The counselor needed to be able to communicate at least minimally facilitative levels of empathic under- standing, respect, genuineness, and concreteness to a client if constructive growth was to occur. This investi- gation, however, used only one dimension of helper be- havior for training. Trainees and supervisors both questioned if this skill was sufficient in the helping process. They seemed to feel that empathic understanding was not enough. Many trainees eXpressed a need for train- ing in additional helping behaviors, such as assertiveness, confrontation, genuineness, and self-disclosure. Archer (1971) found that undergraduate para— professionals using an integrated IPR training model could train other undergraduates to have more effective inter- Personal skills. The author's data indicated that with aPPI'Opriate training and supervision, undergraduates could function as paraprofessional group leaders for interpersonal 150 skills groups. In addition to these findings, Archer found that undergraduate "helpers" could become important re- ferral agents for students in need of long-term counseling assistance. The undergraduate paraprofessional might also become a crucial intervention source in the dormitory or college setting. While lay helpers might not be apprOpri- ate for students in need of long-term therapy, these para- professionals might provide short-term helpful services to the students in the dormitory and college. Implications for Future Research The results of this study seemed to raise several questions which could be considered in future research. A replication comparative study should be carried out using the same procedures with other samples of under- graduate helpers. Other sources of these "helpers" include resident assistants from other dormitories, student tutors, and undergraduate volunteer workers in the city's social agencies. The efficacy of individual training of lay pro- fessionals in facilitative communication versus group training of paraprofessionals had not previously been tested in the Carkhuff model. Training in communication skills was usually conducted in small groups. Future studies might consider the effects of individual training also. 151 Comparative studies are still needed to investi- gate the functioning of professional helpers as compared to the levels of paraprofessional behavior. This need for research is emphasized by the results of this study, which questioned the Carkhuff contention that parapro- fessionals functioned at levels equal to (but never less than) professional helpers. The procedures used in this study were logically planned by the experimenter. Other investigations are needed to examine the ordering of training tasks in both the Carkhuff and Kagan models. The influence of a high or low functioning inquirer on trainee growth seemed to warrant consideration. Pierce and Schauble (1970) described the possible influ- ence of supervisor functioning on trainee growth. But the impact of the inquirer's level of functioning had not been investigated in the Kagan model. Additional studies might investigate the effec— tiveness of a 40-hour training program conducted over a longer period of time. For instance, the efficacy of a 40-hour (two-month) training program in helping behaviors Could be compared to an intensive 40-hour training program Of five days (see Dendy, 1971). Other measures of helping behaviors which may be learned by trainees need to be defined, operationalized, and used as dependent variables in future studies. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Anthony, W. A., & Carkhuff, R. R. Effects of training on rehabilitation counselor trainee functioning. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 1970, 13, 333—342. — Archer, James, Jr. Undergraduates as paraprofessional leaders of interpersonal communication skills training groups using an integrated IPR (Inter- personal Process Recall) videotape feedback/ affect simulation training model. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. Armstrong, J. C. Perceived intimate friendship as a quasi-therapeutic agent. Journal of Counseling Psyphologyp 1969, pp, 137-141. Berenson, B. G., Carkhuff, R. R., & Myrus, P. The inter- personal functioning and training of college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1966, p3, 441—446. Berenson, B. G., Mitchell, K. M., & Moravec, J. A. Level of therapist functioning, patient depth of self-exploration and type of confrontation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, pp, 136—139. Brown, W. F. Student-to-student counseling for academic adjustment. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 2, 811-8170 Cabush, David W. Outcome effects of training college stu- dent clients in facilitative self-responding. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. EXperimental and quasi- experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand, McNally and Co., 1963. 152 153 Carkhuff, R. R. Differential functioning of lay and professional helpers. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, pp, 117-126. Carkhuff, R. R. Toward a comprehensive model of facili- tative interpersonal processes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1967, pp, 67-72. Carkhuff, R. R. Training in the counseling and thera- peutic practices: Requiem or reveille? Journal of Counseling Psychology: 1966, pp, 360-367. Carkhuff, R. R., & Bierman, R. Training as a preferred mode of treatment of parents of emotionally disturbed children. Journal of Counselipg Psychology, 1970, pp, 157-161. Carkhuff, R. R., & Griffin, A. H. The selection and train- ing of human relations specialists. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1970, pp, 433-450. Carkhuff, R. R., Krathochvil, D., & Friel, T. Effects of professional training: Communication and discrimi— nation of facilitative conditions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, pp, 68-74. Carkhuff, R. R., & Truax, C. B. Lay mental health counseling: The effects of lay group counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology: 1965, 22, 426- 431. Carkhuff, R. R., & Truax, C. B. Toward explaining success and failure in interpersonal learning eXperiences. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1966, pp, 723-728. Carkhuff, R. R., & Truax, C. B. Training in counseling and psychotherapy: An evaluation of an integrated didactic and experiential approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1965, pg, 333-336. Cooley, W. W., & Lohnes, P. R. Multivariate data anapysis. New York: Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1971. Danish, S. J., & Brodsky, S. L. Training of policemen in emotional control and awareness. American Psycho- Danish, S. J., & Kagan, N. Emotional simulation in counseling and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 1969, p, 261-263. 154 Dendy, R. F. A model for the training of undergraduate residence hall assistants as paraprofessional counselors using videotape playback techniques and interpersonal process recall. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. Ebel, R. L. Estimates of the relatibility of ratings. In W. A. Henrens & R. L. Ebel (Eds.), Principles of educational and psychological measurement. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1967. Goldberg, A. D. A sequential program for supervising counselors using the interpersonal process recall technique. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1967. Greenberg, B. S., Kagan, N., & Bowes, J. Dimensions of empathic judgments of clients by counselors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969, pp, 303-308. Grzegroek, A. E. A study of the effects of two types of emphasis in counselor training used in conjunction with simulation and videotaping. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. Harvey, L. V. The use of non-professional auxiliary counselors in staffing a counseling service. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1964, 348-351. Hays, W. L. Statistics for psychologists. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1963. Hoyt, C. J. Test reliability estimated by analysis of variance. In W. A. Mehrens & R. L. Ebel (Eds.), Princpples of educational and psychological measurement. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1967. Hummel, T. J., & Sligo, J. R. Empirical comparison of univariate and multivariate analysis of variance procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 1971, pp, 49-570 Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. R., & Miller, R. Stimulated recall in therapy using video tape--a case study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1963, pg, 237- 243. 155 Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. R., Goldberg, A. D., Campbell, R. J., Schauble, P. G., Greenberg, B. 8., Danish, S. J. Resnikoff, A., Bowes, J., & Bondy, S. B. Studies in human interaction: Interpersonal process recall stimulated by videotape. East Lansing: Educational Publication Services, 1967. Kagan, N., & Schauble, P. G. Affect simulation in inter- personal process recall. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969, pp, 309-313. Kagan, N., Schauble, P., Resnikoff, A., Danish, S., & Krathwohl, D. Interpersonal process recall. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1969, pgg, 365-374. Kell, B. L., & Mueller, W. J. Impact and change: A study of counseling relationships. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966. Kirk, R. E. Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences. Belmont, Calif.: Brooks/ Cole Publishing Co., 1969. Magoon, T. A., & Golann, S. E. Nontraditionally trained women as mental health counselors/psychotherapists. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1966, pp, 788-793. Martin, J. C., Carkhuff, R. R., & Berenson, B. G. Process variables in counseling and psychotherapy: A study of counseling and friendship. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1966, pp, 356-359. McArthur, C. G. Comment on "effectiveness of counselors and counselor aides." Journal of Counseling Ppychology, 1970, pp, 335-336. Morrison, D. F. Multivariate statistical methods. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1967. Patterson, C. H. Theories of counseling and psychotherapy. New York: Harper and Row, 1966. Pierce, R. M., & Drasgow, J. Teaching facilitative inter- personal functioning to psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969, pp, 295- 298. Pierce, R. M., & Schauble, P. G. Graduate training of facilitative counselors: The effects of indi- vidual supervision. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1970, pp, 210-215. 156 Resnikoff, A., Kagan, N., & Schauble, P. G. Acceleration of psychotherapy through stimulated videotape recall. American Journal of Psyphotherapy, 1970, pp, 102-111. Rioch, M. J., Elkes, C., Flint, A. A., Usdansky, B. S., Newman, R. G., & Silber, E. National Institute of Mental Health pilot study in training mental health counselors. American Journal of Ortho- psychiatpy, 1963, 33, 678-689. Robinson, F. P. The dynamics of communication in counsel- ing. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1955, p, 163-169. Rogers, C. R. The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Con- sulting Psychology, 1957, pp, 95-103. Schauble, P. G. The acceleration of client progress in counseling and psychotherapy through interpersonal process recall. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Schlossberg, N. K. Sub-professionals: To be or not to be. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, p, 108-113. Schmidt, L. D. Comment on "differential functioning of lay and professional helpers." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, pp, 127-129. Spivack, J. D. The use of developmental tasks for training counselors using interpersonal process recall. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Truax, C. B., & Carkhuff, R. R. Toward effective counsel- ing and psychotherapy: Training and practice. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company, 1967. Truax, C. B., & Lister, J. L. Effectiveness of counselors and counselor aides. Journal of Counseling PgYChOlOgXI 1970’ ll, 331-3340 Truax, C. B., Carkhuff, R. R., & Douds, J. Toward an integration of the didactic and experiential approaches to training in counseling and psycho- therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1964, ‘pp, 240-247. 157 Woody, R. H., Krathwohl, D. R., Kagan, N., & Farquhar, W. W. Stimulated recall in psychotherapy using hypnosis and video tape. The American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1965, 1, 234-241. Zunker, V. G., & Brown, W. F. Comparative effectiveness of student and professional counselors. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1966, pp, 738-743. APPENDICES APPENDIX A THE EMPATHIC UNDERSTANDING IN INTERPERSONAL PROCESSES SCALE AND RATING FORM APPENDIX A THE EMPATHIC UNDERSTANDING IN INTERPERSONAL PROCESSES SCALE AND RATING FORM Empathic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes, II A Scale for Measurement1 Robert R. Carkhuff Level 1 The verbal and behavioral expressions of the first person either do not attend to or detract significantly from the verbal and behavioral expressions of the second person(s) in that they communicate significantly less of the second person's feelings than the second person has communicated himself. Examples: The first person communicates no awareness of even the most obvious, may be bored or dis- interested or simply operating from a precon- ceived frame of reference which totally excludes that of the other person(s). 1The present scale "Empathic understanding in inter- personal processes" has been derived in part from "A Scale for the measurement of accurate empathy" by C. B. Truax which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized in Truax & Carkhuff, 1967) and in part from an earlier version which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized in Carkhuff & Berenson, 1967). In addition, similar measures of similar constructs have received extensive support in the literature of counseling and therapy and education. The present scale was written to apply to all interpersonal 158 159 In summary, the first person does everything but express that he is listening, understanding or being sensitive to even the feelings of the other person in such a way as to detract significantly from the communications of the second person. Level 2 While the first person responds to the expressed feelings .of the second person(s), he does so in such a way that he subtracts noticeable affect from the communications of the second person. Examples: The first person may communicate some awareness of obvious surface feelings of the second person but his communications drain off a level of the affect and distort the level of meaning. The first person may communicate his own ideas of what may be going on but these are not con- gruent with the expressions of the second person. In summary, the first person tends to respond to other than what the second person is expressing or indicating. Level 3 The expressions of the first person in response to the expressed feelings of the second person(s) are essentially interchangeable with those of the second person in that they express essentially the same affect and meaning. Example: The first person responds with accurate under- standing of the surface feelings of the second person but may not respond to or may misinterpret the deeper feelings. processes and represent a systematic attempt to reduce the ambiguity and increase the reliability of the scale. In the process many important delineations and additions have been made, including in particular the change to a syste- matic focus upon the additive, subtractive or interchange- able aspects of the levels of communication of understand- ing. For comparative purposes: Level 1 of the present scale is approximately equal to Stage 1 of the Truax scale. The remaining levels are approximately correspondent: Level 2 and Stages 2 and 3 of the earlier version; Level 3 and Stages 4 and 5; Level 4 and Stages 6 and 7; Level 5 and Stages 8 and 9. The levels of the present scale are approximately equal to the levels of the earlier version of this scale. 160 The summary, the first person is responding so as to neither subtract from nor add to the expressions of the second per- son; but he does not respond accurately to how that person really feels beneath the surface feelings. Level 3 consti- tutes the minimal level of facilitative interpersonal functioning. Level 4 The responses of the first person add noticeably_to the expressions of the second person(s) in such a way as to express feelings a level deeper than the second person was able to express himself. Example: The facilitator communicates his understanding of the expressions of the second person at a level deeper than they were expressed, and thus enables the second person to experience and/or express feelings which he was unable to express previously. In summary, the facilitator's responses add deeper feeling and meaning to the eXpressions of the second person. Level 5 The first person's responses add significantly to the feel- ing and meaning of the expressions of the second person(s) in such a way as to (l) accurately express feelings levels below that the person himself was able to express or (2) in the event of ongoing deep self-exploration on the second person's part to be fully with him in his deepest moments. Examples: The facilitator responds with accuracy to all of the person's deeper as well as surface feelings. He is "together" with the second person or "tuned in" on his wavelength. The facilitator and the other person might proceed together to eXplore previously unexplored areas of human existence. In summary, the facilitator is responding with a full awareness of who the other person is and a comprehensive and accurate empathic understanding of his most deep feel- ings. RATER NO. 161 SUBJECT NO. Empathic Understanding 1 2 3 4 5 Section 1 Section 2 Section 3 APPENDIX B THE IPR COUNSELOR VERBAL RESPONSE SCALE AND RATING FORM IPR COUNSELOR VERBAL RESPONSE SCALE The scale was develOped as a part of a project supported by a grant from the U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Office of Education, "Exploration of the Potential Value of Interpersonal Process Recall Technique (IPR) for the Study of Selected Educational Problems" (Project Nos. 7-32-0410-216 and 7-32-0410-270). 162 163 IPR COUNSELOR VERBAL RESPONSE SCALE The Counselor Verbal Response Scale is an attempt to describe a counselor's response to client communication in terms of four dichotomized dimensions: (a) affect- cognitive; (b) understanding-nonunderstanding; (c) specific- nonspecific; (d) exploratory-nonexploratory. These dimen- sions have been selected because they seem to represent aspects of counselor behavior which seem to make theo- retical sense and contribute to client progress. The unit for analysis is the verbal interaction between counselor and client represented by a client state- ment and counselor response. A counselor response is rated on each of the four dimensions of the rating scale, with every client-counselor interaction being judged inde- pendently of preceding units. In judging an individual response the primary focus is on describing how the counse- lor responded to the verbal and nonverbal elements of the client's communication. Description of Rating Dimensions I. Affect-cognitive dimension The affective-COgnitive dimension indicates whether a counselor's response refers to any affective component of 164 a client's communication or concerns itself primarily with the cognitive component of that communication. A. Affective Responses--Affective responses generally make reference to emotions, feelings, fears, etc. The judge's rating is solely by the content and/or intent of the counselor's response, regardless of whether it be reflection, clarification, interpretation. These responses attempt to maintain the focus on the affective component of a client's communication. Thus they may: (a) Refer directly to an explicit or implicit reference to affect (either verbal or nonverbal) on the part of the client. . Example: "It sounds like you were really angry at him." (b) Encourage an expression of affect on the part of the client. Example: "How does it make you feel when your parents argue?" (c) Approve of an expression of affect on the part of the client. Example: "It doesn't hurt to let your feelings out once in a while, does it?" (d) Presents a model for the use of affect by the client. Example: "If somebody treated me like that I'd really be mad." Special care must be taken in rating responses which use the word "feel." For example, in the statement "Do you feel that your student teaching experience is helping you get the idea of teaching?", the phrase "Do you feel that" really means "do you think that." Similarly the expression "How are you feeling?" is often used in a matter-of-fact, conversation manner. Thus, although the verb "to feel" 165 is used in both these examples, these statements do not represent responses which would be judged affective." B. Cognitive Responses--Cognitive responses deal primarily with the cognitive element of a client's com- munication. Frequently such responses seek information of a factual nature. They generally maintain the interaction on the cognitive level. Such responses may: (a) Refer directly to the cognitive component of the client's statement. Example: "So then you're thinking about switching your major to chemistry?" (b) Seeks further information of a factual nature from the client. Example: "What were your grades last term?" (c) Encourage the client to continue to respond at the cognitive level. Example: "How did you get interested in art?" II. Understanding-nonunderstanding dimension The understanding-nonunderstanding dimension indi- cates whether a counselor's response communicates to the client that the counselor understands or is seeking to understand the client's basic communication, thereby encouraging the client to continue to gain insight into the nature of his concerns. A. Understanding responses--Understanding re- sponses communicate to the client that the counselor under- stands the client's communication--the counselor makes apprOpriate reference to what the client is expressing or trying to express both verbally and nonverbally—-or the 166 counselor is clearly seeking enough information of either a cognitive or affective nature to gain such understand- ing. Such responses: (a) Directly communicate an understanding of the client's communication. Example: "In other words, you really want to be treated like a man." (b) Seek further information from the client in such a way as to facilitate both the counselor's and the client's understanding of the basic problems. Example: "What does being a man mean to you?" (c) Reinforce or give approval of client communications which exhibit understanding. Example: CL: "I guess then when people criticize me, I'm afraid they'll leave me." CO: "I see you're beginning to make some connection between your behavior and your feelings." B. Nonunderstanding responses--Nonunderstanding responses are those in which the counselor fails to under- stand the client's basic communication or makes no attempt to obtain appropriate information from the client. In essence, nonunderstanding implies misunderstanding. Such responses: (a) Communicate misunderstanding of the client's basic concern. Example: CL: "When he said that, I just turned red and clenched my fists." CO: "Some peOple don't say nice things." (b) Seek information which may be irrelevant to the client's communication. Example: CL: "I seem to have a hard time getting along with my brothers." CO: "Do all your brothers live at home with you?" 167 (c) Squelch client understanding or move the focus to another irrelevant area. Example: CL: "I guess I'm really afraid that other peOple will laugh at me." CO: "We're the butt of other peOple's jokes sometimes." Example: CL: "Sometimes I really hate my aunt." CO: "Will things be better when you go to college?" III. Specific-nonspecific dimension The Specific—nonspecific dimension indicates whether the counselor's response delineates the client's problems and is central to the client's communication or whether the response does not specify the client's con— cern. In essence, it describes whether the counselor deals with the client's communication in a general, vague, or peripheral manner, or "zeros in" on the core of the client's communication. NB: A response judged to be nonunderstand- ing must also be nonspecific since it would, by definition, misunderstand the client's communication and not help the client to delineate his concerns. Responses judged under- standing might be either specific (core) or nonSpecific (peripheral) i.e., they would be peripheral if the counse- lor conveys only a vague idea that a problem exists or "flirts" with the idea rather than helping the client delineate some of the dimensions of his concerns. A. Specific responses--Specific responses focus on the core concerns being presented either explicitly or implicitly, verbally or nonverbally, by the client. Such responses: (a) (b) (c) 168 Delineate more closely the client's basic concerns. Example: "This vague feeling you have when you get in tense situations-—is it anger or fear?" Encourage the client to discriminate among stimuli affecting him. Example: "Do you feel in all your classes or only in some classrooms?" Reward the client for being specific. Example: CL: "I guess I feel this way most often with someone who reminds me of my father." CO: "So as you put what others say in perspective, the whole world doesn't seem so bad, it's only when someone you value, like Father, doesn't pay any attention that you feel hurt." B. Nonspecific responses--Nonspecific responses indicate that the counselor is not focusing on the basic concerns of the client or is not yet able to help the client differentiate among various stimuli. Such responses either miss the problem area completely (such responses are also nonunderstanding) or occur when the counselor is seek- ing to understand the client's communication and has been presented with only vague bits of information about the client's concerns. Thus such responses: (a) (b) Fail to delineate the client's concern and cannot bring them into sharper focus. Example: "It seems your problem isn't very clear-- can you tell me more about it?" Completely miss the basic concerns being presented by the client even though the counselor may ask for specific details. Example: CL: "I've gotten all A's this year and I still feel lousy." CO: "What were your grades before then?" 169 (c) Discourage the client from bringing his concerns into sharper focus. Example: "You and your sister argue all the time. What do other peOple think of your sister?" IV. Exploratory—Nonexploratory dimension The eXploratory-nonexploratory dimension indicates whether a counselor's response permits or encourages the client to eXplore his cognitive or affective concerns, or whether the response limits a client's exploration of these concerns . A. Exploratorygresponses--Exploratory responses encourage and permit the client latitude and involvement in his response. They may focus on relevant aspects of the client's affective or cognitive concerns but clearly attempt to encourage further exploration by the client. Such re— sponses are often open-ended and/or are delivered in a manner permitting the client freedom and flexibility in response. These responses: (a) Encourage the client to explore his own concerns. Example: Cognitive--"You're not sure what you want to major in, is that it?" Affective-~"Maybe some of these times you're getting mad at yourself, what do you think?" (b) Assist the client to explore by providing him with possible alternatives designed to increase his range of responses. Example: Cognitive--"What are some of the other alternatives that you have to history as a major?" Affective-—"In these situations do you feel angry, mad, helpless, or what?" 170 (c) Reward the client for exploratory behavior. Example: Cognitive--"It seems that you've considered a number of alternatives for a major, that's good." Affective--"So you're beginning to wonder if you always want to be treated like a man." B. Nonexploratory responses——NoneXploratory re— sponses either indicate no understanding of the client's basic communication, or so structure and limit the client's responses that they inhibit the exploratory process. These responses give the client little opportunity to explore, eXpand, or express himself freely. Such responses: Discourage further exploration on the part of the client. Example: Cognitive-—"You want to change your major to history." Affective--"You really resent your parents treating you like a child." 171 when Imuonxm Icoz anon IMHonxm onuflommm mcflocmum lumps: Icoz mcflbcmum lumps: m>Huflcmou m>fluomuu¢ mmmcommwm "mama "pomnnsm Mdflom UZHBfim mmzommmm Aflmmm> MOAWmZDOU mmH “wmpsn APPENDIX C HELPEE SELF-EXPLORATION IN INTERPERSONAL PROCESSES SCALE APPENDIX C HELPEE SELF-EXPLORATION IN INTERPERSONAL PROCESSES SCALE Helpee Self-Exploration in Interpersonal Processes, II A Scale for Measurement1 Level 1 The second person does not discuss personally relevant material, either because he has had no opportunity to do such or because he is actively evading the discussion even when it is introduced by the first person. Example: The second person avoids any self-descriptions or self-exploration or direct expression of feel- ings that would lead him to reveal himself to the first person. In summary, for a variety of possible reasons, the second person does not give any evidence of self—exploration. 1The present scale "Self-exploration in inter- personal processes" has been derived in part from "The measurement of depth of intrapersonal exploration" (Truax, 1963) which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965, 1965a, 1965b; Rogers, 1962; Truax, 1963; Truax & Carkhuff, 1963, 1964, 1965). In addition, similar measures of similar constructs have received extensive sup- port in the literature of counseling and therapy (Blau, 1953; Braaten, 1958; Peres, 1947; Seeman, 1949; Steele, 1948; Wolfson, 1949). The present scale represents a systematic attempt to reduce the ambiguity and increase the reliability of the scale. In the process many important delineations and 172 173 Level 2 The second person responds with discussion to the intro- duction of personally relevant material by the first per- son but does so in a mechanical manner and without the demonstration of emotional feeling. Example: The second person simply discusses the material without exploring the significance or meaning of the material or attempting further exploration of that feeling in our effort to uncover related feelings or material. In summary, the second person responds mechanically and remotely to the introduction of personally relevant material by the first person. Level 3 The second person voluntarily introduces discussions of personally relevant material but does so in a mechanical manner and without the demonstration of emotional feeling. Example: The emotional remoteness and mechanical manner of the discussion give the discussion a quality of being rehearsed. In summary, the second person introduces personally rele- vant material but does so without spontaneity or emotional proximity and without an inward probing to newly discover feelings and experiences. Level 4 The second person voluntarily introduces discussions of personally relevant material with both spontaneity and emotional proximity. Example: The voice quality and other characteristics of the second person are very much "with" the feel- ings and other personal materials which are being verbalized. additions have been made. For comparative purposes, Level 1 of the present scale is approximately equal to Stage 1 of the earlier scale. The remaining levels are approxi- mately correspondent: Level 2 and Stages 2 and 3; Level 3 and Stages 4 and 5; Level 4 and Stage 6; Level 5 and Stages 7, 8, and 9. 174 In summary, the second person introduces personally rele- vant discussions with spontaneity and emotional proximity but without a distinct tendency toward inward probing to newly discover feelings and experiences. Level 5 The second person actively and spontaneously engages in an inward probing to newly discover feelings and experiences about himself and his world. Example: The second person is searching to discover new feelings concerning himself and his world even though at the moment he may be doing so, perhaps, fearfully and tentatively. In summary, the second person is fully and actively focusing upon himself and exploring himself and his world. APPENDIX D ROLE AND FUNCTION OF THE INQUIRER APPENDIX D ROLE AND FUNCTION OF THE INQUIRER Ideally, the recaller should remain as neutral as possible, and avoids forming a new relationship with the person being interrogated. His function is to help the person discover for himself some of his feelings and thoughts which interfered with effective communication. Often the recaller will gain insight before the person being interrogated but the recaller should lead the person to discover for himself what was happening. In effect, the recaller should avoid telling the person what was happening. He should avoid making judgments and interpreting for the other person. He should gently probe and push for more material with a direct line of questioning with only occasional use of reflective statements. It takes time for the person to gain insight so the recaller should be wary of a need to get the job done quickly. The recaller should focus on the feelings of the person being interrogated--i.e., the feelings the person was having about himself and the other person during the videotaped interaction. A suggested line of questioning might be the following: 1. What do you think he was trying to say? 2. What do you think he was feeling at this point? 3. Can you pick up any clues from his nonverbal behavior? 4. What was running through your mind when he said that? 5. Can you recall some of the feelings you were having then? 6. Was there anything that prevented you from sharing some of your feelings and concerns about the person? 7. If you had another chance would you like to have said something different? 8. What kind of a risk would there have been if you said what you really wanted to say? 175 176 9. What kind of a person do you want him to see you as? 10. What do you think his perceptions are of you? The recaller should encourage the person being interrogated to stop the machine as often as he wants. He should also reinforce the person as much as possible--i.e., just before starting the machine again, say, "you're doing good--stop the machine whenever you recall some of the feelings you were having." Suggested Rotation for the 3 Roles Speaker Helper Recaller Trainee O‘U‘IDWNI-J i—‘ChUW-bOJN UJNI—‘ChU'lb Each videotaped interaction, recall session, and group discussion will take approximately 30 min. In a 3-hour period everyone will have one turn at each of the 3 roles. APPENDIX E TRAINER'S MANUAL--ITM-I TRAINING PROCEDURES APPENDIX E TRAINER'S MANUAL--ITM-I TRAINING PROCEDURES Kathy Scharf Tom Spierling Bob Dendy Trainer's Manual--Facilitative Conditions and IPR Day l--Monday, September 14, 1970 Time: l-2:30 P.M. I. Discrimination of Client (Speaker) Self-Exploration in Interpersonal Processes—~11 (Ex) Purpose: Introduce basic concepts of the Ex scale and discrimination of the five levels of Ex. A. Trainees will work in groups of six with a trainer B. Trainees practice discriminating client (speaker) self—exploration by rating speaker statements from an audio-taped protocol (Part I). Rate each client (speaker) statement at one of the five levels of speaker self-exploration. Refer to: "Client Self-Exploration in Interpersonal Processes--II"--Carkhuff (Appendix I of this manual) Time: 3-5:00 P.M. II. Discrimination of Counselor (Listener) Empathetic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes—-II (E) 177 178 Purpose: Introduce basic concepts of the E scale and discrimination of the five levels of E. A. Trainees will work in groups of six with a trainer. B. Trainees rate audiotaped responses to client state- ments (Part II of the audiotaped protocol). Dis- cuss each rating focusing on the elements of the response that make it a level 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5 response. (The audiotaped protocol includes a client statement with four (4) counselor responses to that statement). Refer to: "Empathic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes--II"—-Carkhuff (Appendix II in this manual) Day 2-—Tuesday, September 15, 1970 Time: 9-10 A.M. III. Empathy training Purpose: Teach trainees to formulate their own responses in terms of speaker feeling and content of speaker statement. A. Trainees will work in groups of six with a trainer. B. Trainees practice responding to audiotaped client statements with empathic understanding. Avoid asking questions such as, "Well, how does that make you feel?" or "You're pretty upset, aren't you?" As a minimum, try to repeat or reflect what the other person said using positive statements. 1. First statement on audiotaped protocol (Part III): Write a response. Try to make the response a positive, reflective statement rather than a question. Trainer will give an average group rating rather than individual ratings. Group discusses the responses. If necessary, rewrite responses in an effort to move closer to level 3, 4, and 5. (Play back speaker if necessary.) 179 2. Second and Third Statements: Write response--same procedure as #1 3. Fourth statement: Respond verbally without writing first. Time: 10:30-Noon V. Mini-Practicum--Part I Purpose: To model exercises which will be included in the practicum. A. Model training exercise 1. Two trainee volunteers needed--one the speaker, one the listener. 2. Trainees interact and videotape the inter- action (ten minutes). Instructions to the trainee-listener (counselor)--"Relate to the person you will be seeing as you would ordi- narily do in order to be helpful to him." Instructions to the trainee-speaker (client)-- "All of us either in the present or during the past year have had a number of experiences which have been very difficult for us. If you feel the person whom you will be seeing is helpful, please feel free to discuss these experiences." B. Inquirer training--trainer models the inquirer role--interrogate the listener (20 minutes). Role of the Recaller—Inquirer The recaller should be as neutral as possible. He is to help the person discover for himself some of the feelings and thoughts which may have interfered with effective communication. The recaller should avoid forming a new relationship with the person being interrogated and instead focus upon the videotaped interaction. Often the recaller will gain insight before the person being interrogated but the recaller should lead the person to discover for himself what was happening. That is, the recaller should avoid making judgments and interpreting to the other person. He should avoid telling the other person what (in his perception) was happening. He, instead, should gently probe and push for more material with a direct line of questioning with only an occasional use of reflective statements. The recaller should let the person being interrogated take the time he needs to gain insight. 180 The recaller should focus on the feelings of the person being interrogated--i.e., the feelings the per— son was having about himself and the other person dur- ing the videotaped interaction. A suggested line of questioning might be the following: A. What do you think he was trying to say? B. What do you think he was feeling at this point? C. Can you pick up any clues from his non- verbal behavior? D. What was running through your mind when he said that? E. Can you recall the feelings you were having then? F. Was there anything that prevented you from sharing some of your feelings and concerns about the person? G. If you had another chance would you like to have said something different? H. What kind of a risk would there have been if you said what you really wanted to say? I. What kind of a person do you want him to see you as? J. What do you think his perceptions are of you? The recaller should encourage the person being interrogated to stop the machine as often as he wants. He should also reinforce the person as much as possi- ble--i.e., just before starting the machine again, say, "You're doing good—-stop the machine whenever you recall some of the feelings you were having." Time: l-3 P.M. V. Mini-Practicum——Part II Purpose: To provide trainees with experience as a helper and with feedback about their own facilitative behaviors. A. Trainees divide into groups of three (l-the listener, l-the speaker, 1-the inquirer). B. Interaction exercise 1. First speaker and listener interact for 10 minutes. (Instructions to speaker and listener the same as outlined previously). Videotape the interaction. Rest of the group observe. C. Time: V. 3. 181 Trainee-inquirer conduct the recall session with the listener (20-30 minutes). (Trainer supervise that the role of the interrogator stays neutral.) Review inquirer role if neces- sary. Trainer and trainees discuss complete session with the Speaker, listener, and interrogator. Repeat B#l-3 for second group of three trainees. 3:30-5:00 P.M. Mini-Practicum (cont.) A. C. Trainees divide into new groups of three (new roles and new pairs, i.e., speakers choose either listener or interrogator role; listeners choose either interrogator or speaker role, etc.) Interaction exercise 1. 2. 3. Speaker and listener interact for 10 minutes. Videotape the interaction. Trainee interrogator conduct recall session of the speaker. Rest of group observes. Trainer and trainees discuss the complete session with the speaker, listener and interrogator. If time allows, repeat for pair 2. Day 3--Wednesday, September 17, 1970 Same schedule as Tuesday. Day 4--Thursday, September 18, 1970 Time: V. Purpose: A. 9-10:30 ll-Noon Mini-Practicum--Part III To provide trainees with experience similar to their future job experience and feedback about their own facilitative behaviors Interviews with a coached client 1. Trainees divide into groups of two (one the listener, one the inquirer). 182 2. Trainee A conducts a lO-minute interview with a coached client. Interview is videotaped. (Instructions to the speaker and listener are the same as previously outlined.) Group observes. 3. Trainee B conducts recall session with both the speaker and the listener simultaneously (IPR Mutual Recall)--20 minutes. 4. Trainer and observer discuss the complete session with the speaker, listener, and inquirer. B. Repeat A#l—4 for trainees 3 and 4. C. Repeat A #1-4 for trainees 5 and 6. Time: l-2:3O P.M. 3—4:3O P.M. Mini—Practicum (cont.) D. l. Trainees form new pairs (reverse roles, i.e., inquirer becomes speaker, speaker is inquirer). 2. Repeat A#2-4 for new trainee pair 1. 3. Repeat A#2-4 for all trainee pairs. Day 5--Friday, September 18, 1970 Same procedure as Thursday. APPENDIX F TRAINER'S MANUAL ITM-II TRAINING PROCEDURES APPENDIX F TRAINER'S MANUAL ITM-II TRAINING PROCEDURES Kathy Scharf Tom Spierling Robert Dendy Trainer's Manual--Facilitative Conditions Day 1--Monday, September 14, 1970 Time: l-2:30 P.M. I. Discrimination of Client (Speaker) Self-Exploration in Interpersonal Processes-II (Ex) Purpose: Introduce basic concepts of the Ex scale and discrimination of the five levels of Ex. A. Trainees will work in groups of six with a trainer. B. Trainees practice discriminating client (speaker) self-exploration by rating speaker statements from audio-taped protocol (Part I). Rate each client (speaker) statement at one of the five levels of speaker self-exploration. Refer to: "Client Self—Exploration in Inter- personal Processes—-II"--Carkhuff (Appendix I of this manual). Time: 3-5:OO P.M. II. Discrimination of Counselor (Listener) Empathic Under- standing in Interpersonal Processes--II (E) Purpose: Introduce basic concepts of the E scale and discrimination of the five levels of E. 183 184 Trainees will work in groups of six with a trainer. Trainees rate audiotaped responses to client statements (Part II of audiotaped protocol). Discuss each rating focusing on the elements of the response that make it a level 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5 response. (The audiotaped protocol includes a client statement with four (4) counselor responses to that statement.) Refer to: "Empathic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes--II"-—Carkhuff (Appendix II in this manual) Day 2, Tuesday, September 15, 1970 Time: III . 9-10:00 A.M. Empathy training Purpose: Teach trainees to formulate their own responses in terms of speaker feeling and content of speaker statement. Trainees will work in groups of six with a trainer. Trainees practice responding to audiotaped client statements with empathic understanding. Avoid asking questions such as "Well, how does that make you feel?" or "You're pretty upset, aren't you?" As a minimum, try to repeat or reflect what the other person said using positive statements. 1. First statement: Write a response. Try to make the response a pgstive, reflective state- ment rather than a questIEn. Trainee will give an average group rating rather than individual ratings. Group discusses the responses. If necessary, rewrite responses in an effort to move closer to level 3, 4, and 5. (Play back speaker if necessary.) 2. Second and Third Statement: Write response-- same procedures #1. 3. Fourth + statements: Respond verbally without writing first. Time: IV. 185 10:30-Noon Introduction to Mini—Practicum Purpose: A. B. Time: V. Model training exercises which will be in- cluded in the practicum. Two trainee volunteers are needed--one the speaker and the other the listener. 1. Trainees interact and audiotape the lO-minute interaction. (Counselor)—-"Re1ate to the person you will be seeing as you would ordi- narily do in order to be helpful to him." Instructions to the trainee—speaker (client)-- "All of us either in the present or during the past year have had a number of experiences which have been very difficult for us. If you feel the person whom you will be seeing is helpful, please feel free to discuss these experiences." Model feedback session 1. Trainee group (observers, speaker listener) rate audiotaped interaction. Listen to three three-minute interactions and rate each segment. Use E scale to rate speaker behavior. 2. Trainer and trainees discuss ratings with the speaker and listener. l-3:00 P.M. Mini-Practicum (cont.) Purpose: To provide trainees with experience in facilitative conditions. To provide trainees with feedback about their own facilitative behaviors. Trainees divide into groups of two (l-the speaker, l-the listener) Pair l--Interaction 1. First speaker-listener pair interact for 10 minutes. (Instructions to the speaker and the listener are the same as Tuesday A.M.) Audio- record the interaction. The rest of the group observe. 3. 186 Trainee group (speaker, listener, and ob- servers) rate the audiotaped interactions (three three-minute segments using the E scale for the listener and the Ex scale for the speaker). Trainer and trainees discuss ratings with the speaker and listener. C. Repeat B#l-3 for pair 2. D. Repeat B#l-3 for pair 3. Time: 3:30-5:00 P.M. V. Mini-Practicum (con't.) Purpose: To provide trainees with experience in facilitative conditions and to provide trainees with feedback about their own facilitative behaviors. A. Trainees divide into new groups of two (new roles and new pairs, i.e., speakers take listeners' role; listeners take speakers' role; new pairing of speaker and listener). B. Pair 1 1. 2. 3. Speaker and listener interact for 10 minutes. Audiorecord the interaction. Group rates the interaction (three three— minute segments) using the E scale for the listener and the Ex scale for the speaker. Group discusses the ratings with the speaker and listener. Day 3, Wednesday, September 16, 1970 V. Mini-Practicum Same schedule as Tuesday Day 4, Thursday, September 17, 1970 Time: 9-10:30, 11:00-Noon V. Mini-Practicum--Part III Purpose: B. C. Time: V. Day 187 To provide trainees with experiences somewhat similar to their future job experience. To provide feedback about trainee facilitative behaviors. Interviews with a coached client. 1. 3. Trainee 1 conducts a lS—minute interview with a coached client. Interview is audiotaped. (Instructions to speaker and listener the same as outlined on Tuesday.) Trainee Group ob- serves. Observers, listener, speaker rate five three- minute segments of the interview using the E scale for the listener and the Ex scale for the speaker. Trainer and trainees discuss the ratings with the speaker and the listener. Repeat A#1-3 for trainee 2. Repeat A#l-3 for trainee 3. 1-2:30 3-4:30 P.M. Mini-Practicum (con't.) D. E. F. Repeat A#l-3 for trainee 4. Repeat A#l—3 for trainee 5. Repeat A#l-3 for trainee 6. 5, Friday, September 18, 1970 Mini—Practicum Same schedule as Thursday. APPENDIX G MEMO TO PROFESSIONAL COUNSELORS APPENDIX G MEMO TO PROFESSIONAL COUNSELORS November 30, 1970 TO: All Senior Staff and Interns FROM: Kathy Scharf and Bob Dendy RE: Dissertation.Desiderata and Data We need volunteers to: a. Conduct 2 half-hour interviews with a student provided by us. One interview will take place during the week of Dec. 7-11 (finals week) and the other during the week of Jan. 4-8. b. Take the Kagan Affective Sensitivity Scale during the same weeks. The scale takes approximately 1 hour. Please return this form to either of our mail boxes by Friday, December 4th if you would like to help us out. 1. Available for interview M T W TH F Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 2. Available for test M T W TH F Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 Name 188 APPENDIX H RELIABILITY RAW DATA nmm.a mmm.mv momma mmouom umumu mom M Nam.a vvm.mm HHH.N mmm.mm mmm.H nam.mm cmmz pcmuw 189 mvv.a ooo.om mm.a oo.mH mm.H oo.aa hw.a oo.vm ma mmm.H ooo.Hm om.m oo.mm 5H.H oo.om mm.H oo.ov Ha omm.a www.mm na.m oo.mm om.H oo.om oo.m oo.mv 0H nmm.m www.5v nm.m oo.mm hm.a oo.~m nm.m oo.mm m noa.m ooo.mm oo.m oo.mm oo.m oo.om om.N oo.om m hvv.m ooo.om 5H.m oo.mm oo.m oo.Hm SH.N oo.om n mmo.m mmm.mm mm.m oo.mm mm.a oo.mm oo.m oo.mm m mmm.H www.ma om.a oo.HH oo.H oo.m om.H oo.Nm m noo.a oo.mm mm.a oo.nH no.a oo.om oo.~ oo.hm v nmm.a mmm.mm om.H oo.mm hm.a oo.vm om.H oo.om m mmm.mm mm.me om.m oo.vv oo.N oo.mm hH.N oo.am m nmm.a mmm.mm mm.m oo.Hm mm.H oo.mm mm.H oo.mv H Dm mm>U Dm mm>U Dm mm>U Dm mm>U Hmnfisz mama new m m umumm N Hmumm H umumm mama mumo 3mm wuHHAQMHHmm H.m OHQMB APPENDIX I THE AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE APPENDIX I THE AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE Instructions You will be viewing short scenes of actual counseling sessions. You are to identify what feelings the clients have toward themselves and toward the counselors they are working with. Although in any one scene a client may exhibit a variety of feelings, for the purpose of this instrument you are to concentrate on identifying his last feelings in the scene. On the following pages are multiple choice items consist— ing of three responses each. Most scenes have two items, but a few have one or three items. After you view each scene, you are to read the items and ask yourself the following question: If the client were to View this same scene, and if he were completely epen and honest with himself (i.e., if he could Identify his real feelings) which of these three responses would he use to describe his feelings? After you decide which response accurately describes what the client is actually feeling either about himself or the counselor he is with, indicate your choice on the answer sheet. Here is a sample item: CLIENT I Scene 1 Item 1 1. This eXploring of my feelings is good. It makes me feel good. 190 191 2. I feel very sad and unhappy. 3. I'm groping and confused; I can't bring it all together. After you had viewed Scene 1 for CLIENT I, you would read these three statements (Item 1) and would then decide which one best states what the client would say about his own feelings after viewing the same scene. For example, if you decide number two best states what the client is feeling, you would then find the number 1 on your answer sheet and darken in the space for number two. 1. l ==== 2 -— 3 ===== 4 :==: 5 ===:-_ We will only make use of the first three answer spaces following each item on your answer sheet. Remember you are to concentrate on the latter part of each scene in determining the most accurate description of the client's feelings. After you view the apprOpriate scenes, you will have 39 seconds to answer each of the first twelve items. For each of the remaining items, you will be allowed 29 seconds. CAUTION: The item numbers on your answer sheet go across the page, not down the page as you would usually expect! AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE REVISED FORM B CLIENT I Scene 1 Item 1 l. I feel sorry for my husband and the relationship we have. 2. I don't really understand what I feel. Yet, I do feel guilty about creating pain in others which returns to me. 3. I feel pleased at seeing a possible relationship between my feelings of anger and pain. Item 2 1. He (counselor) doesn't have to like me. I just want him to agree with me and tell me I'm right. 2. I'm trying to please you. Do you like me? 3. He's really understanding me now. 192 CLIENT I Scene 2 Item 3 1. I feel calm and collected. I just want to think for a while. 2. Yes, that is when I get angry. I see it all clearly now. 3. I feel anxious and stimulated. Item 4 1. I'll pretend I'm agreeing with him (counselor), but I don't see the connection at all. 2. I like what he's doing. I don't feel as un- comfortable now. 3. I wish he would stop pushing me in this direction. CLIENT II Scene 1 Item 5 1. I'm leased, happy; I feel good all over! 2. It was brought right back, that amazes me, but it hits quite bad too. It hurts! 3. I'm not bothered by this. I can handle it. I'm confident. Item 6 1. He's (counselor) caught me; careful, I'm not sure I want that. 2. I like him. He's trying to make the situation a little lighter and made me feel better about it. 3. I don't feel he understands. He's sarcastic. I don't like that. CLIENT II Scene 2 Item 7 l. I feel a little uneasy and self-conscious, but not much. 2. This scares me. I feel frightened! 3. I feel flirtatious. I like this! Item 8 l. I feel a little bit embarrassed, but that's all right as long as I can keep my composure. 2. I have a feeling of sadness. 3. I feel flustered and embarrassed. 193 Item 9 l. 2. He's asking for some touchy material, but that's all right. It's about time he knew. He's being very frank and open! I'm not sure I want that. 3. I want him to leave me alone-—I want out of here. I don't like this. CLIENT II Scene 3 Item 10 1. I'm getting so much attention. I really enjoy this. It makes me feel good. 2. I'm scared by what I'm feeling. I feel embarrassed and threatened. 3. I have the feeling that what I wanted was wrong, and I'm a little ashamed of myself. Item 11 1. This is good. We're really moving into my feelings. 2. He's too perceptive; he's looking right through me. 3. He's getting a little sticky; I'm not sure I like that. CLIENT III Scene 1 Item 12 l. I feel protective and defensive of what people may think about my family. 2. All this seems so pointless! I'm puzzled and bored. 3. We're having a nice conversation. Some of these things really make me think. Item 13 1. This guy (counselor) embarrasses me with the questions he asks. 2. The questions he asks really make me think. I'm not sure I like that. 3. I can't follow this guy's line of thought. What's he trying to do? 194 CLIENT IV Scene 1 Item 14 1. I'm concerned about my physical condition. I'm worried about it. 2. I want pity. I want her to think "Oh, you poor boy." 3. I feel good-—nothing's bothering me, but I enjoy talking. Item 15 1. She's too young to be counseling, and she's a girl. I'm not sure I like this. 2. She likes me; I know she does. 3. I'd like her to think I'm great. CLIENT IV Scene 2 Item 16 1. I'm a little annoyed with my family's ambitions for me. 2. That's a hell of a lot to ask! It makes me mad! 3. I feel sorry for myself, and I want others to feel the same. Item 17 1. She (counselor) really understands me! She's with me now. 2. I don't feel much either way towards the counselor; she's not important to me. 3. I wonder if she appreciates the pressure that's put on me? CLIENT IV Scene 3 Item 18 1. This whole thing just makes me feel sad and unhappy. 2. It kind of angers me that they don't appreciate me when I feel I did my best. I wish I could tell them off. 3. No matter how well I do, I'm always criticized. It doesn't bother me too much though because I know that I did my best. 195 Item 19 l. I can tell she understands what I'm saying. She's really with me. 2. I wish I could get out of here; I don't like her. 3. Understand what I'm saying; I want her to know how I feel. CLIENT IV Scene 4 Item 20 1. I really want to be successful, and somehow I know that I can be. 2. That makes me feel kind of sad, unhappy. I don't want to believe that it's true--I want to be good. 3. I don't know what I feel here. It's all very confusing. Item 21 l. I feel neutral towards her here. I'm not paying any attention to her. 2. Please feel sorry for me and try to help me. I wish she would praise me. 3. I like talking to her. She can be trusted even to the point of telling her how I really feel about myself. CLIENT V Scene 1 Item 22 l. I feel rejected and empty inside. Am I unloveable? 2. I feel a little lonely. I want my boy friend to pay a little more attention to me. 3. I really don't feel much here; I'm just kind of talking to fill up space. Item 23 1. Please say it isn't fair, Mr. Counselor. 2. He really understands me. I can tell him anything. 3. I'm not sure I care what he says. It's kind of unimportant to me what he feels about me at this time. 196 CLIENT V Scene 2 Item 24 1. I'm afraid of marriage--insecure; it might not work out, and I'd be lost. 2. I really can give him all the affection he needs, I feel I'm a worthwhile person to be desired. He wouldn't dare step out on me. 3. I'm really not too worried; it'd all work out in the end even if we have to go to a marriage counselor. Item 25 l. I don't care if he (counselor) can help me or not. I'm not sure I want his help. 2. He's so sympathetic. That makes me feel good. 3. Can you help me? CLIENT V Scene 3 Item 26 l. I feel I have some need to be liked, but it's not real strong. 2. I'm not loveable; I don't really like myself. 3. I'm a good person; I'm loveable. Down deep I know I am. Item 27 l. I feel dejected, kind of insecure. I want to be likeable! 2. My main concern is that it's hard for me to take criticism. I usually think of myself as perfect. 3. I feel a little sad about all this; I do kind of want people to like me. Item 28 1. He thinks well of me; I know he does, I can tell. 2. I want the counselor to really like me, but I'm not sure he does. 3. I like it when he asks questions like that. They make me really think about deeper things. 197 CLIENT V Scene 4 Item 29 l. I wouldn't want to be treated like he treats Mother, but I don't mind him (stepfather) too much. 2. I feel very little emotion about anything at this point. 3. I hate him (stepfather)! Item 30 1. Boy, I'm happy that he (counselor) agrees with me. He sympathizes with me. I feel completely accepted. 2. I'm embarrassed to tell the counselor how strong my feelings really are. 3. I'm not sure he'll be able to help me much after all. I'll just have to work this out by myself. CLIENT V Scene 5 Item 31 1. I'm kind of feeling sorry for myself, but I'm not really too worried. 2. I want to move out of the house as soon as possible. I feel I would be better off on my own. 3. My own parents don't want me; I feel cut off and hurt. Item 32 l. I don't feel he's (counselor) helpful at all, and if he can't help me and see my side, I'm not going to like him either. 2. He's got me in a spot, but I feel I can still get him to see me as a good girl who is persecuted. 3. I wish the counselor were my father. He's listening; he understands how I feel. CLIENT VI Scene 1 Item 33 l. Disapprove! She'd kill me! 2. I feel jovial; this is real interesting. 3. I'm not sure how she would feel but the whole idea of her finding out excites me. 198 Item 34 1. He (counselor) understands me completely. He certainly is relaxed and comfortable. 2. I really don't care what he feels about me. I just want someone to talk to--anyone will do. 3. I was wondering how he would feel about me and what I'm saying. CLIENT VI Scene 2 Item 35 l. I think my brother is O.K. We have fun together. 2. I don't know what I'm saying here. I'm a little mixed up and confused. 3. I'm saying something that's important to me. I like Doug. CLIENT VI Scene 3 Item 36 1. This is very confusing for me. I'm not sure I understand what is going on. 2. This is how I really feel, I'm kind of starting to be myself. 3. I'm just talking to be talking here; this really doesn't mean much to me. Item 37 l. I guess he's (counselor) all right, but I'm still not sure he understands me. 2. Let's get going. I'm impatient! I want to move to more important matters. 3. I feel comfortable with him. He understands me. CLIENT VI Scene 4 Item 38 l. I love my brother, but not romantically. We just have a good brother-sister relationship. 2. I don't know about feeling this way about Doug; it feels so good, but it concerns me too. 3. I feel better about my relationship with Doug now. It helps to get it out in the open. Now I feel it's all right. 199 CLIENT VI Scene 5 Item 39 1. I'm not feeling much of anything here. I'm just kind of talking to be talking. 2. I'm mad at everyone at this point and don't know which way to turn; I guess I'm mad at myself too. 3. Now I'm talking about things that are real. I'm not on stage anymore. She is a louse! Item 40 1. He (counselor) feels she's a bad person too. I can tell; he agrees with me. 2. Don't you agree with me? I want to know what you think. 3. He thinks this all sounds petty. He doesn't understand. CLIENT VII Scene 1 Item 41 1. I felt angry with my mother, but this made me feel guilty. I needed to make an excuse for her. 2. I'm really not angry with mother. It's not her fault. 3. I'm in a very passive mood. I'm just relaxing and talking about things that interest me. Item 42 1. This counselor is all right. I feel I can confide in him. 2. I feel uncomfortable. I'm not sure what this counselor wants me to do. 3. I feel he wants me to talk about myself, but I don't care. I'm going to talk about what I want to talk about. CLIENT VII Scene 2 Item 43 1. I'm very sensitive; I'm very easily hurt. 2. I'm somewhat sensitive and easily hurt, but not deeply so. 3. I'm not sensitive or easily hurt at all. I just like to make people think I am. 200 Item 44 1. That makes me mad, I can do it--I know I can, but things just keep getting in my way. 2. It's really all his fault, if he just wouldn't have been such a joker. 3. This makes me feel guilty; I need to blame someone else instead of blaming myself. Item 45 1. I'm neutral towards the counselor. I don't care what he feels about me. 2. I'm afraid he doesn't like me and what I'm saying about myself. I don't want him to be harsh with me. 3. He's easy to talk to. He understands what I'm like, and he still likes me. I can confide in him. CLIENT VIII Scene 1 Item 46 1. Say, this is all right. I like this. 2. I'm not feeling anything deeply. I know what I need! 3. It's embarrasing and difficult. I feel a little annoyed. Item 47 l. I feel I can rely on this guy, so I'll let him talk and I'll just answer his questions. 2. I wonder what you think about this-~please respond. Give me some help! 3. The counselor is a good guy. I like his questions; they make it easier for me. CLIENT VIII Scene 2 Item 48 1. I feel very unhappy about what I may eventually have to do. 2. I don't know what I feel; I'm confused about what I feel. 3. I'm damned uncomfortable; it's so confusing. I feel kind of 'blah' about it all. 201 Item 49 1. He's (counselor) missing the point. He bugs me. 2. I can't really tell about this guy. I don't know how I feel about him. 3. He seems like a good guy. He asks nice questions. I like him. CLIENT IX Scene 1 Item 50 1. I'm not sure how I feel about this counselor. I don't feel one way or the other about him. 2. I like the counselor very much-~he makes me feel good. 3. He understands me pretty well and is trying to help. I guess I kind of like him. CLIENT IX Scene 2 Item 51 l. Goody, goody peeple don't really know any better, so I can't be too disgusted with them, but it does make me angry. 2. I don't really mind people feeling superior to me. It just makes me a little angry. 3. It tears me up inside when peeple think they're better than I am. I want people to be the same as me. Item 52 1. I'm every bit as good as they are. I really feel I am. I know I am. 2. I kind of wished they liked me, but I can live without being a member of their group. 3. Those smart kids make me feel stupid. Item 53 l. I feel sorry for them; they just don't realize what they're doing to people like me. 2. I feel I'm not as good as they are, and it really hurts when people act that way. 3. It makes me a little angry. I'm every bit as good as they are. 202 CLIENT IX Scene 3 Item 54 l. I feel a little insignificant, and this makes me a little unhappy. 2. I'm a nobody. I'm always left out. 3. I'm unhappy with school. That's what is really bothering me. Item 55 1. He (counselor) doesn't quite understand, but I don't care. It doesn't matter. 2. I don't feel one way or the other towards this counselor, we're just having a nice talk. 3. He (counselor) is really listening to me, and I feel he understands what I'm feeling. CLIENT X Scene 1 Item 56 1. I'm feeling scared, concerned. Is this for me? 2. I just feel uncertain about what to talk about. If I once get started, I'll be all right. 3. I feel very deeply depressed. Item 57 1. He (counselor) seems to be listening—-can he understand how I feel? 2. He's really with me. I can tell he understands me. 3. He doesn't keep things moving enough. I don't like that. CLIENT X Scene 2 Item 58 1. I'd like to think I could make it, but I'm not sure. I feel inadequate. 2. I just have an I-don't-care feeling; that's my real attitude towards all of this. 3. I'm confused here. I really don't have any definite feelings. Item 59 l. I want to impress the counselor. I want him to believe I can do it. 2. He believes me; he thinks I can do it; I can tell. 3. I really don't care what the counselor thinks. It's not important to me. 203 CLIENT X Scene 3 Item 60 1. What's the use of looking ahead? I'm scared to think about it. 2. I can accept my situation. Really, things aren't so bad. Things may bother me a little, but really not much. 3. I enjoy just living for today. Item 61 1. He's (counselor) all right. He really under- stands me. 2. Nobody can really understand this. I don't think he will be any different. 3. I don't care what he thinks or feels; he's not important to me anyway. CLIENT X Scene 4 Item 62 l. I feel somewhat unhappy. I don't like to feel this way. 2. There's something about me; I just don't fit in, and that makes me feel real inadequate. 3. In some instances, I'm unsure of myself. I'm afraid I'll do the wrong thing, but I can handle this just by avoiding these situations. CLIENT XI Scene 1 Item 63 1. I'm unhappy about all this, but I'm afraid to make a change. 2. It's not that I don't like school, it's just that I want to do the things I like most. 3. I'm not the student type. School bores me, but it embarrasses me when I say it. Item 64 l. The counselor is a nice guy. I like him, and I think he likes me. . 2. I wonder what the counselor thinks of me. He'll probably think less of me for saying this. 3. I don't care what he thinks of me. It doesn't really matter to me. 204 CLIENT XI Scene 2 Item 65 1. I've found some new dimensions. I like to feel that I can have some excitement, but this kind of scares me too. 2. This doesn't really mean much. I'm not feeling much of anything. 3. This makes me feel very guilty; I'm very ashamed. Item 66 l. I suppose he'll (counselor) tell me that's wrong, too. I'm not sure he understands me very well. 2. He's O.K.; he's listening to what I have to say. He really understands me and my feelings. 3. I don't care what he thinks or feels; it's not important. I don't have any feelings towards the counselor. CLIENT XI Scene 3 Item 67 1. He's really with me; he understands just how I'm feeling. 2. I'm not concerned about what he feels or thinks about me. It doesn't matter to me one way or the other. 3. I'm afraid of what he'll think or feel about what I'm saying. "1111!!!!"11111111“