it: IS .3‘ .112 NNERSITY LIBRARIES rm; illit’llllifili; llllil‘lll 3 1293 01015 4155 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Role formation within hierarchical decision making teams with distributed expertise: A role expansion model presented by Douglas J. Sego has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. d . Organizational Behavior egreeln c. (”f/MM Major professor Date 7////9‘% MS U is an Affirmative Action /Eq ual Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LIBRARY Mlchlgan State University PLACE N RETURN BOX to monthl- chockout from your record. 1'0 AVOID FINES rotum on or baton date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU IsAn Afflnnativo MUM“ Opportmlty Initiation mm: _..._.—__—_.- _ ,_ ROLE FORMATION WITHIN HIERARCHICAL DECISION MAKING TEAMS WITH DISTRIBUTED EXPERTISE: A ROLE EXPANSION MODEL By Douglas James Sego A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fiIlfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Management 1 994 ABSTRACT ROLE FORMATION WITHIN HIERARCHICAL DECISION MAKING TEAMS WITH DISTRIBUTED EXPERTISE: A ROLE EXPANSION MODEL By Douglas James Sego The purpose of this study was to examine the determinants of role expansion. Three groups of factors were studied, leadership (consideration and initiating structure), individual (experience) and situational (feedback). In addition, role expansion was also examined as a predictor of individual decision making accuracy. Two additional predictors of decision making accuracy were Stress and job knowledge. Subjects (n = 240) were studied within four person teams using the TIDE2 computer simulation and assigned to one of the four experimental conditions. The two by two design include experience (operationalized as experiencing one or three jobs within the team) and type of feedback (operationalized as outcome feedback only or outcome and cognitive feedback). In addition, the effects of job knowledge and stress on decision making accuracy was examined. Experience was found to have a Significant negative impact on role expansion while the leader behavior of consideration had a significant positive impact. Both stress and experience had Significant negative effects on individual decision making accuracy. Implications for future research are discussed. © Copyright by Douglas James Sego l 994 Dedicated to my children Heather, Joshua and Clarence iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS There are a number of individuals who helped me to this point, where graduate school ends and my third professional life begins. The individuals mentioned below never gave up on me even when I thought I would never finish. Their support, guidance, and friendship will always be remembered. First, I would like to thank all of the graduate students from the Organizational Behavior and Industrial/Organizational programs. Particularly, those who worked in the Team Effectiveness Lab with me, Jen Hedlund, Jean Phillips, Chuck Capps, Dave Waldschmidt, Dale Tuttle, Spence Tower, Linda Barrett and especially Debra Major. They made those long hours of data collection not only bearable, but frequently quite fiJn. Collectively, there were the best and brightest colleagues and friends one could hope for. Special thanks to Anne O'Leary-Kelly who always was there when I needed someone to believe in me. Finally, thanks to Samatha Wolf, a very Special friend, who's will to survive will always help me keep my life in perspective. Of the faculty members who were a part of my education, I want to mention five. My first research project was with Steve Kozlowski. His guidance and help when I was completely lost was never forgotten. Alison Barber, who agreed to be on my committee V at the 13th hour. I wish she had joined the faculty earlier so I may have had an opportunity to work with her. Daniel Ilgen, whom I have the greatest respect for. As a scholar and researcher his work speaks for its self. Most importantly, he always gave me the most honest feedback about my strengths and weaknesses. Kevin Ford, who over the years has been a teacher, mentor, critic, card partner, best man, and friend. His dedication to his profession will always inspire me. John Hollenbeck, who brought me into the program and was a mentor and friend at every stage right up to the end, including being my committee chair. He did not give up on me and in the end was instrumental in my finishing my program. I can not hope to match his achievements, but his example will always be my goal. Most importantly I would like to thank my wife, Virginia Unkefer. Her encouragement lead me to apply to graduate school and her faith in my abilities helped me to finish. Together we have faced the hardest time of our lives. I only hope I can be as supportive for her. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................... ix LIST OF FIGURES ................................... x Chapter One: Introduction ............................... 1 Chapter Two: Roles ................................... 5 Overview .................................... 5 History of Role Theory ............................. 6 Classical Role Theory .............................. 8 NPI Role Theory ................................ 11 Vertical Dyadic Linkage Theory ......................... 12 Job/Role Differentiation Theory ......................... 15 Role Expansion ................................. 19 Leadership and Substitutes for Leaders ..................... 23 Research on Task Experience .......................... 26 Research on Feedback .............................. 28 Theoretical Model ................................ 30 Model of Role Expansion ............................ 31 Summary .................................... 47 Chapter Three: Method Section ............................. 49 Subjects ..................................... 49 TIDE2 Simulation ................................ 49 Research design ................................. 53 Manipulations and measures ........................... 54 Experience ................................ 54 Feedback ................................ 55 Leader characteristics .......................... 61 Role Expansion ............................. 62 Stress .................................. 66 Role incumbent knowledge ....................... 66 Team decision making accuracy ..................... 66 Procedures ................................ 66 Data Analysis .................................. 69 vii Primary data analysis ........................... 69 Secondary data analysis ......................... 69 Power Analysis ............................. 69 Chapter Four: Results .................................. 73 Analysis of the Role Expansion Hypotheses ................... 73 Hypothesis 1 ............................... 73 Hypothesis 2 ............................... 73 Hypothesis 3 ............................... 73 Hypothesis 4 ............................... 78 Hypothesis 5 ............................... 78 Hypothesis 6 ............................... 78 Hypothesis 7 ............................... 78 Hypothesis 8 ............................... 78 Analysis of the Decision Making Accuracy Hypotheses ............. 80 Hypothesis 9 ............................... 80 Hypothesis 10 .............................. 84 Post Hoc Analysis of Decision Making Accuracy Data ............. 84 Chapter Five: Discussion ................................ 87 Role Expansion ................................. 87 Unsupported Hypotheses ........................ 88 Supported Hypothesis .......................... 91 Significant results in the opposite direction ................ 91 Decision making accuracy ........................... 92 Unsupported hypotheses ......................... 93 Significant findings ............................ 94 Limitations ................................... 94 Future Research ................................. 96 Conclusion ................................... 97 APPENDIX A: Coding instructions for leader/incumbent communications ....... 98 APPENDIX B: Handbook for command and control simulation ............ 99 APPENDD( C: Job Knowledge Test ......................... 105 APPENDIX D: Interactive Training Script ...................... 117 APPENDIX E: Debriefing Sheet: Experiment 7 ................... 121 BIBLIOGRAPHY .................................. 122 viii LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Inter-rater reliability for the leader consideration and initiating structure scales and text message coding ........................ 63 Table 2: Internal consistency for the task knowledge test across all four conditions. . .67 Table 3: The timing and order of procedures used in the experiment .......... 68 Table 4: Data analysis plan for the regression equation where role expansion is the dependent variable ............................ 70 Table 5: Data analysis plan for the regression equation where team decision making performance is the dependent variable ..................... 71 Table 6: Means and standard deviations of variables in the study ............ 74 Table 7: Intercorrelations among the variables in the study ............... 75 Table 8: Regression results with role expansion as the dependent variable ....... 76 Table 9: Regression results with decision making accuracy as the dependent variable . .81 Table 10: Post hoc regression results with decision making accuracy as the dependent variable .............................. 85 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Classical role theory model (Katz & Kahn, 1978) ............... 9 Figure 2: Vertical dyadic linkage role model (Graen & Scandura, 1987) ........ 13 Figure 3: Job/Role differentiation theory model (Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1992) ...... 17 Figure 4: Text message content coding scheme ..................... 24 Figure 5: Role expansion model ............................ 32 Figure 6: The moderating effects of experience on the leader's behavior of consideration to incumbent role expansion relationship ............ 36 Figure 7: The moderating effects of experience on the leader's behavior of initiating structure to incumbent role expansion relationship .......... 38 Figure 8: The moderating effects of feedbaCk on the leader's behavior of consideration to incumbent role expansion relationship ............ 40 Figure 9: The moderating effects of feedback on the leader's behavior of initiating structure to incumbent role expansion relationship ......... 41 Figure 10: Incumbent and task characteristics as moderators of the role expansion to task performance relationship ................. 43 Figure 11: The moderating effects of task knowledge on the role expansion to task performance relationship ...................... 45 Figure 12: Figure 13: Figure 14: Figure 15: Figure 16: Figure 17: Figure 18: Figure 19: Figure 20: Figure 21: Figure 22: Figure 23: The moderating effects of stress on the role expansion to task performance relationship ..................... Weights and values settings for the TIDE2 simulation (screen 1) ..... Weights and values settings for the TIDE" simulation (screen 2) . . . . Process feedback form for the carrier position .............. Process feedback form for the CAD position ............... Process feedback form for the AWACS position ............. Process feedback form for the cruiser position .............. Main effect of the leader's behavior of consideration on role expansion . . Main effect of job experience on role expansion .............. Interaction between the leader's level of initiating structure and the type of feedback received on role expansion ............... Main effect for stress on decision making inaccuracy performance . . . . Main effect for experience on decision making inaccuracy performance. . . .46 .51 . .52 .57 .58 .59 .60 .77 .79 .82 .83 .86 Chapter One: Introduction Research on small groups has a history dating back before World War H. Over the decades the focus of small group research has often changed. The change is due in part to less research conducted by social psychologists and more research conducted by organizational psychologists (Levine and Moreland, 1990). Organizational psychologists are for the most part reacting to the realities in the work place. The popular press is fill] of stories of organizations downsizing, reductions in middle management, and the restructuring work around work groups or self regulating teams. The last decade has seen an increase in the amount of research conducted on teams (Ilgen, Major, Hollenbeck, & Sego, 1993) and in particular on decision making teams (Davis, Holt, Spitzer, & Stasser, 1981; Sniezek & Henry, 1989). Two reasons contribute to this increased interest. First, there are a number of dramatic examples of decision making team failures. In the case of the USS Stark (1987), the decision making team failed to take action that would have placed the ship in a stronger position to defend itself from the Iraqi war plane. In the case of the USS Vincense (1988), the decision making team failed to interpret the available data correctly and shot down an Iranian civilian airliner. Finally, in the case of the Bureau of Alcohol, Tobacco and Fire Arms (ATF) in Waco, Texas (1993), the decision making team relied on the judgments of experts in the areas of law, religion, psychology and military tactics. The result was a decision to take the Branch Davidian compound by force, a decision that will be questioned for a long time. A second reason for the increased interest in teams has to do with the challenges 1 2 facing businesses today. The business environment is becoming ever more global and, as a result, organizations are having to deal with increasing complexity in the workplace and greater global competition. This has resulted in organizations needing to find ways to decrease Operating costs while, at the same time, increasing productivity and effectiveness. Teams are often implemented in organizations to achieve these goals. To decrease costs, many organizations are becoming flatter in their corporate structures. As levels of management and the amount of supervision are being reduced, workers are gaining greater control over their own work (Raynor, 1992) often as members of teams (Solomon, 1991; Jacob, 1992). Teams can be found in many difl‘erent types of industries, such as the airline (Foushee, 1984), the military (Rouse, Cannon-Bowers, & Salas, 1991), the automobile (Fortune, 1992; Moskal, 1992), and the consumer goods industries (Jacob, 1992). Current research on teams looks into several issues related to teams including decision making accuracy (Guzzo, 1986; Stevenson, Busemeyer, & Naylor, 1990), training (Cannon-Bowers & Salas, 1990), and communication (Kleinman, 1990). One area not currently receiving much attention is how decision making team members define and develop their individual roles. Understanding how roles develop in decision making teams is important for two reasons. First, incumbents of decision making teams are different from the type of incumbents that are Often studied in role research. Instead of the role being defined as a set of observable behaviors that incumbents engage in to perform a task, incumbents in decision making teams use information to perform their tasks. Although there may be little difl‘erence between these two groups, there is no research on roles in decision making teams to base a comparison on. Second, incumbents in decision making teams are often highly skilled in Specific content areas. This high level of specialized competency is important to the team's overall performance in making decisions. However, specialized competency can create situations where team members have very different interpretations of a single event or situation. Resolving this conflict between team members may create new problems. One possible solution is for the team members to learn portions of each other's role so that they can more clearly see how each piece of information fits together to create a whole. However, how much sharing of individual team member's roles can take place before the advantages of specialized expertise are compromised? Another question is: what are the consequences on performance when specific roles are shared by more than one team member? To answer these questions it is important to understand what factors effect role development, and then to look at how the role development of individual team members effects their decision making performance. Although several different types of teams have emerged, one of the most ubiquitous types is the hierarchical decision making team with distributed expertise. The central defining features of this type of team are the following. First, the primary purpose of this team is to collect and share information, and to make judgments or decisions based on an analysis of the information. Decision making team members come together either face-to-face or through technology mediated communication to perform their jobs. Second, the leader is accountable for the decisions made by this type of team. Occasionally, such as in juries, anonymity within the decision making process is important. However, within most situations in organizations, accountability is desirable. Third, team 4 members have distributed expertise to help cope with the increased complexity of work. Flexible manufacturing maintenance teams (Bergstrom, 1992), multi-functional design teams (Jacob, 1992) and hostage rescue teams (Ling, May 3, 1993) are examples of teams that require individuals with highly specialized expertise to come together for a common purpose. The purpose of this research was to test antecedents of the role development process, in hierarchical decision making teams with distributed expertise. This study looked at characteristics of the leader, and the role incumbent, as well as situational factors and how they effect the way incumbents develop their roles. In addition, factors of the incumbent and situation are posited as substitutes for behaviors of the leader in the role definition process. A secondary purpose of the study was to look at the impact of role development on decision making performance. Two predicted moderators of this relationship are incumbent job knowledge and task related Stress. Chapter Two: Roles Overview The purpose of this chapter will be to review several literatures relevant to role theory and the model of the role development process presented later. The first section provides a historical overview of role research across several different research domains. The second section reviews classical role theory as developed by Katz and Kahn (1978). The focus here will be on the role sender influence in the role definition process. The third section reviews Naylor, Pritchard, and Ilgen's (1980) theory of roles. The focus of this theory is on the role incumbent's influence in the role development process. Both Katz and Kahn (1978) and Naylor, Pritchard and Ilgen (1980) suggest that the leader is influential in the developing of subordinate's roles. Thus several theories of leadership that are important to this investigation of roles are reviewed. The fourth section reviews the vertical dyadic linkage (VDL) literature. VDL is framed as a leader- incumbent negotiation model. It describes the negotiation process where roles are defined. The fifth section reviews the recent attempt to integrate the job and role literature by Ilgen and Hollenbeck (1991). Their job role differentiation theory extends VDL by focusing on what is negotiated and why. The sixth section takes a closer look at the construct of role expansion which was introduced in the preceding section. Role expansion will be discussed within the framework of the incumbent, whose job is to make decisions. The seventh section briefly reviews the leadership and substitutes for leadership literatures. Most role theories place the supervisor or leader in a central position in the role definition process. Consequently, possible alternatives for the leader's behavior are proposed within 5 6 the role expansion process. These alternatives include previous experiences and the type of task feedback. The eighth section reviews the experience literature. This literature Often views experience as a Single dimension. Experience will be shown to be multidimensional and these other dimensions are important when looking at experience's impact on role development. The ninth section reviews the feedback literature. The focus of this review will be on the differences between outcome feedback and cognitive feedback. The final section develops a theoretical model of the role expansion process. This will include predictions that incumbent, situational and leader characteristics have direct effects on the roles expansion process. The model also predicts a direct efi‘ect between incumbent role expansion and task performance moderated by incumbent and Situational characteristics. History of Role Theory Role theory research has a long history reaching back to 19403 and 19503 (Biddle, 1979). As with most fields of study, much of the early theoretical work attempted to define what roles were and to describe the boundary conditions that existed when defining roles. Biddle states that some theorists conceived roles as primarily overt behaviors (Davis, 1949) or covert processes (N ewcomb, 1950). On the other hand, roles have also been theorized as learning behaviors and thus controlled by internal variables (Child, 1963) or controlled by others (Cattell, 1963). Most of this early work on roles was done by sociologists and social psychologists. In a recent review of the role literature, Biddle (1986) listed five perspectives of role theory. The first is functional role theory which believes roles are shared norms that are found in stable social systems. Two shortcomings of this theory are the requirements 7 of shared norms and stable social systems. These two requirements lack credible evidence, according to Biddle. The second theory is symbolic interactionist role theory that places the focus on individual actors. Here roles are thought to represent attitudes, contextual demands and norms as understood by the actor or role incumbent. Although this perspective also includes the concept of norms, it does so while recognizing that it is the actor's interpretation and not the norm that is important. The third role theory is structural role theory. Structural role theory focuses on stable organizations of sets of persons sharing the same set Of behaviors. This theory places the importance of understanding roles on the social environment instead of on the individual. By placing little emphasis on understanding individual differences, structural role theory has a modest following. The fourth theory iS cognitive role theory that for the most part focuses on role expectations. Areas of research interest include the conditions that lead to expectations, the measurement of expectations, and the effect of expectations on behavior. Although cognitive role theory has been criticized for relying too much on contemporary American culture, it does have a large empirical research base. The fifth theory is organizational role theory. The focus here is on preplanned social systems that are task oriented and hierarchically organized. Roles, in organizational role theory, are social positions with normative expectations. Organizational role theory includes the concept that norms can vary between individuals holding the same social position and that the influences on individuals include official policies as well as peer pressure. The relationship between role expectations and behaviors is also important in these theories. Organizational role theory will be the framework used here to develop a model of role expansion. The focus is on how individuals expand their roles in hierarchically arranged decision making teams where team members have distributed expertise. The following is a brief review of four theories of roles in organizations that were used as the foundation for the predictions of role expansion made in this study. Classical Role Theory Classical role theory (Katz and Kahn, 197 8) is an example of an organization role theory. Classical role theory is primarily a two-person theory of roles. The two participants are the role sender and the role receiver. The role sender refers to someone who has expectations about what behaviors are appropriate for the role receiver. These expectations include formalized standards for the social position held, such as a job description, and personal expectations the role sender has for the role receiver beyond the job description. The role sender is often a supervisor but can be a co-worker or even a subordinate. The role receiver refers to the person who occupies the organizational position of interest. I will refer to this person as the role incumbent. In Katz and Kahn's role theory, the role definition process starts with the role sender communicating his/her role expectations to the role incumbent. This is represented in Figure 1 by the top line between the role sender and the role incumbent. The sent role is then received and decoded by the role incumbent. In the next step, the role incumbent engages in some behavior that he/she believes meets the role sender's expectation. Finally, the role incumbent's behavior is observed by the role sender who decides if the behavior 9 Figure 1: Classical role theory model (Katz & Kahn, 1978). Role sender Focal person Role Sent expectation role Feedback Perceived Role role behavior 10 matches his/her expectations. If it does not, this process is repeated until either the role incumbent does what is expected or the role sender changes his/her role expectations. Katz and Kahn (1978) state that they: consider role-sending as a continuing cyclical process by means of which each person is socialized into a particular organizational role, informed about the acceptability of his/her behavior in relation to the requirements of that role, and corrected as necessary. (p. 187) Katz and Kahn (1978) note that the role incumbent is not a completely passive participant in the role definition process. When the role incumbent receives the sent role the individual decodes the message and decides how to respond. How the incumbent interprets the message and chooses to respond through behaviors is largely under this person's control. This choice is likely to be effected by the role incumbent's experience, ability, and job knowledge. These act as a filter to the received role expectations and influence the direction of his/her behaviors. Classical role theory is a closed loop system involving two people. Role information is sent, received and acted on. The observable behaviors are then used as feedback and analyzed to determine if the role incumbent fillfilled the role sender's expectations. This process continues until there is no discrepancy between the role sender's expectations and the incumbent's behaviors. This perspective of role sending and role taking places most of the responsibility for defining roles on the role sender, who initiates the role definition process. Consequently, it would be expected that certain leader 11 behaviors would be very influential in the role definition process. NPI Role Theory Another theory of roles in organizations was developed by Naylor, Pritchard and Ilgen (1980), and is typically referred to as NPI theory. Consistent with organizational role theory, these authors describe roles as stable patterns of interdependent behavior from individuals who occupy social positions within an organization. The patterns of behaviors enacted by the role incumbent are central in their theory. Acts are the basic processes of behaviors and are conceptualized as having the dimensions of time and effort. These two dimensions are not easily observed or measured. Consequently, outcome products are used as evidence of role behavior. Two additional requirements are needed for behaviors to be role related according to NPI theory. First, the behavior must be relevant to the role. Top management personnel Often get involved in charity work, such as serving on a local art counsel. However, this may not be relevant to their jobs and thus is not part of their role. Second, the product must be important enough to be evaluated by someone. For instance, a personnel office worker may post the company's stock quote each day so that workers who are vested in company stock can track their investments. However, if this is not part of the worker's job and no one evaluates him/her for doing this, then, according to NPI theory, it is not part of his/her role. Thus if the role behavior outcomes are not relevant to the role or important enough to be evaluated by others, then NPI theory states that the outcomes are not role related. A significant difference exists between NPI theory and classical role theory (Katz and Kahn, 1978). Classical role theory posits that the role sender initiates the role 12 definition process, while NPI theory posits the role incumbent initiates this process. This is an important distinction. NPI theory allows the role incumbent to take a more proactive stance in defining his/her role. In classical role theory, the role incumbent is generally reacting, although with volition, to expectations of a role sender. These two theories offer examples where both the role sender and the role incumbent initiate the role definition process. A commonality is found in that both theories include a negotiation process, where roles are defined during the negotiations that go on between the role sender and role incumbent. The following theory takes a closer look at this negotiation process. Vertfil Dyadic Linkage Theory The Work by Graen and Scandura (Graen, 1976; Graen & Scandura, 1987) looks at the process of defining roles within organizations. Their focus is on dyadic relationships and the negotiation processes that go on between the supervisor and the subordinate (role incumbent). They refer to this process as the vertical dyadic linkage (VDL). VDL is primarily relevant for unstructured tasks that are not easily included in a formal job description. These tasks require that the supervisor and role incumbent negotiate the incumbent's role using a three-stage process that is depicted in Figure 2. The first stage in this process, shown in Figure 2a, is the sampling phase and is very similar to the Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, & Rosenthal (1964) role episode model. The supervisor initiates the process by sending the role incumbent his/her expectations. The role incumbent receives and then acts, responding to supervisor's expectations. The role incumbent's behaviors are then used by the supervisor as feedback and are used to determine if they meet his/her expectations. Graen and Scandura (1987) state that in this 12 definition process, while NPI theory posits the role incumbent initiates this process. This is an important distinction. NPI theory allows the role incumbent to take a more proactive stance in defining his/her role. In classical role theory, the role incumbent is generally reacting, although with volition, to expectations of a role sender. These two theories offer examples where both the role sender and the role incumbent initiate the role definition process. A commonality is found in that both theories include a negotiation process, where roles are defined during the negotiations that go on between the role sender and role incumbent. The following theory takes a closer look at this negotiation process. Vertical Dyadic Linkage Theory The Work by Graen and Scandura (Graen, 1976; Graen & Scandura, 1987) looks at the process of defining roles within organizations. Their focus is on dyadic relationships and the negotiation processes that go on between the supervisor and the subordinate (role incumbent). They refer to this process as the vertical dyadic linkage (VDL). VDL is primarily relevant for unstructured tasks that are not easily included in a formal job description. These tasks require that the supervisor and role incumbent negotiate the incumbent's role using a three-stage process that is depicted in Figure 2. The first stage in this process, shown in Figure 2a, is the sampling phase and is very Similar to the Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, Snoek, & Rosenthal (1964) role episode model. The supervisor initiates the process by sending the role incumbent his/her expectations. The role incumbent receives and then acts, responding to supervisor's expectations. The role incumbent's behaviors are then used by the supervisor as feedback and are used to determine if they meet his/her expectations. Graen and Scandura (1987) state that in this 13 Figure 2: Vertical dyadic linkage role model (Graen & Scandura, 1987). ENVIRONMENT Superior 3. Member STRUCTURE Sent / \ LEADER Role Received EXPECTATIONS A Role Received Response Response \ BEHAVIOR J WORKING RELATIONSHIP Opportunity RESOURCES ”CW“ OIIOI % GHQ! Perceived ACTIVITIES 3°“ Role 4f Role MEMBER '\ EXPECTATIONS / DCR /_ Behavior Perceived POTCO IVOd NOI’I‘TIS ACTIVITIES Behavior NOffl'IS DCR Role Taking Role Making Role Routlnization 14 phase the supervisor acts and the role incumbent reacts. The second phase in Grain and Scandura's role definition model is the role development phase. During this phase the supervisor and role incumbent test out different dyadic interdependencies. This is depicted in Figure 2b. The third phase, the commitment phase, is where the role is routinized (Figure 2c). According to Graen and his colleagues, over time the dyadic relationship between the supervisor and role incumbent is established and becomes relatively stable. Many studies have looked at VDL as a leadership theory in order to explain the relationship between the supervisor and incumbent (Dansereau, Cashman, & Graen, 1973; Vecchio & Gobdel, 1984). The quality of the dyadic relationship reflects whether the incumbent is a member of the supervisor's in-group or out-group. In—group members are generally given more desirable jobs and more latitude in doing their jobs than are out- group members. AS an outcome of the role development phase (where roles are negotiated), some incumbents are allowed to add to their roles while other are not. VDL is important in understanding the process by which incumbents define their roles. First, it Shows how roles are negotiated through the dyadic interactions between a supervisor and a role incumbent. One result of the negotiation process is members being considered in- or out-group members. Second, it demonstrates how different incumbents holding the same job develop different roles. Those incumbents that are in the supervisor's in-group are more likely to develop greater role responsibilities then those in the supervisor's out-group. This results in incumbents with the same job having much difi‘erent roles. The prominence of the supervisor and incumbent dyad has some limitations, 15 however, when it comes to explaining the role definition process. Most other role theories posit that individuals who are not the role incumbent's supervisor can still be influential in defining the incumbent's role (Biddle, 1986; Katz & Kahn, 1978; Naylor et al., 1980). Naylor et al. (1980) refer to this group of individuals collectively as the role incumbent's role set. Katz and Kahn (1978) posit that peers and subordinates can be role senders. Seers (1989) attempts to address the exclusion of non-supervisors in the dyadic negotiation process though the concept of team member exchanges (TMX). TMX is still a role theory that centers around a dyadic relationship. However, TMX allows for individuals other than the supervisor, such as the incumbent's peers, to influence the role development process. In addition, TMX studies the effects of multiple dyadic relationships on each incumbent. This review of the role literature has pointed out important constructs and issues related to the role development process. There still remains an important issue when considering roles and role development. That issue focuses on the distinction between jobs and roles. In the job and role literatures there is ambiguity about the actual boundaries between jobs and roles. The following section will review recent theoretical work that attempts to clear up this ambiguity, first by clearly defining a job and a role and then by stating where the similarities between the two end. Job/Role Differentiation Theory Each person in an organization is responsible for performing a set of duties or tasks that are sometimes called task elements. The task elements define a job and the behaviors that are expected of individuals in a job. Task elements are formalized within the organization in the form of a job description, worker handbook or the intrinsic 16 demands of the tasks themselves. Ilgen and Hollenbeck (1991) define a job as: a set of task elements grouped together under one job title and designed to be performed by a single individual (p. 173). It is important to note that the job is defined by the group of task elements and not by the individuals who occupy the job. There is some confusion between the concepts of jobs and roles. This confusion results in part because a job is at the core of a role. The distinction between a job and a role is that a role is an individual's expansion of his/her job. Figure 3 shows the relationship between a job and role as explained by Ilgen and Hollenbeck's (1991) job/role differentiation theory. The center box represents the task elements that make up the job. In Ilgen and Hollenbeck's view jobs have four basic characteristics: 1) they are created by prime beneficiaries, 2) they are objective, 3) they are bureaucratic and 4) they are quasi static. These characteristics clearly suggest that jobs exist independent of the individuals who occupy them. The definition of a role includes the above definition of a job with the addition of those things that an individual either brings to the job or negotiates to have become part of the job. Ilgen and Hollenbeck (1991) call these emergent role elements. In Figure 3 the role is represented by the outer box. This shows the role encompassing the job. The area between the borders of the two boxes represents the emergent role elements. It is in this area of the role that explains how two incumbents can have the same job but can have considerably different roles. 17 Figure 3: Job/Role differentiation theory model (Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1992). Role Role Emergent Role Job Job Elements 18 Job role differentiation theory is usefiJl in clarifying two issues. First, it clearly defines the distinction between jobs and roles. The role is not unique in comparison to the job but rather is an extension of the job. Second, it expands VDL by explaining two factors. The first factor is what is negotiated in the role definition process (i.e., emergent role elements). The second factor is why emergent role elements need to be negotiated (i.e., to expand the basic job to include those things the organization needs to have accomplished that are not in the actual job description). Job elements are stable over time and are the same across incumbents within a job. Emergent role elements are negotiated between the supervisor and the incumbent according to most role theories. Emergent role elements are necessary for performing a job because of the limitation in defining a job using a job descriptions. In attempting to be precise and parsimonious, a job description cannot be all-inclusive. An example of this problem can be seen during management and labor contract negotiations. Sometimes union officials will threaten to tell their members to do only what is required of them according to their job description. This is a powerfirl threat because so much of what workers do and what the organization needs to have done is not spelled out specifically in the job description but is negotiated and subsequently becomes part of the workers' roles in the form of emergent role elements. Consequently, the workers' roles are expanded beyond their basic jobs to meet the needs of the organization. Role expansion is the process of adding emergent role elements to a set of task elements that make up and define a job. The final result of this process is the worker's role. 19 Role Expansion If jobs are expanded to form roles and two workers are likely to expand their roles in unique ways, then it is important to understand the antecedents of role expansion. What are the antecedents and consequences to the role expansion process? The two parts of this question are explored in this research. There is little or no empirical research in the Industrial/Organizational literature on the role expansion process, with the exception of VDL. Instead, the empirical research on roles has focused on issues such as the impact of conflicting expectation of an incumbent's role (i.e., role conflict) or the uncertainty over exactly what the incumbent's role is (i.e., role ambiguity). Yet there are reasons to look at the process itself. If role expansion can be predicted, then there could be implications for job design. It is possible that different ways of designing jobs could result in incumbents developing different roles. There are also implications for other functions within the organization. When a worker retires, does an organization recruit someone to fill the job as described in the job description or the role that was developed by the newly retired worker? If the new employee does not have the skills or background to fill the emergent task elements of the retiree's role, then how does the organization go about filling this void? Do other workers expand their roles to fill the void or is it the expectation that the new worker will? If the latter is the case, then it will be important for the organization to create the appropriate conditions to help insure that the new employee expands his/her role in a way that meets the organization's needs. Job re-design can also be effected by role expansion. An organization may make changes to a job to increase autonomy (Hackman & Oldham, 1975) because they believe that this will lead to greater job satisfaction. The outcome might be disappointing if no 20 consideration is given to how job incumbents have expanded their roles. It is possible that some or all of the incumbents have created roles that already give them greater autonomy. The result of job re-design might be no increase in autonomy and hence job satisfaction. Consequently, understanding role expansion should be of interest to an organization. What exactly is role expansion? In its simplest form role expansion is the addition of emergent task elements to a person's role. An example of role expansion is an office clerk who learns how to use a new computer graphics software package even though there is no mention of computer graphics in his/her job description. This Skill has utility to the organization because reports can be made with interesting graphics. Using computer graphics is not formally a part of the office worker's job because of the newness of the technology but its use could become part of his/her role. Because most job descriptions do not change with each new change in technology, workers' roles are expanded to include the changes. For the purposes of this study, role expansion is defined as workers adding job relevant task elements to their jobs that go beyond the formal description of their jobs. This definition might suggest that role expansion is a uni-dimensional construct, the simple adding of task elements. However, few constructs are that simple and role expansion is no exception. There are many different types of task elements found in any given job in organizations. Depending on the mix of task elements that are added to a job, the resulting roles within any given job could be quite different. This study will look at one type of job, one with decision making that involves multiple interdependent participants. The decision making job will be the boundary for the following discussion on multiple dimensions of role expansion. 21 The starting point for looking at role expansion is the job the worker holds. If the job is one of making decisions, then the task elements have to do with the gathering, sharing and processing of information. Because processing information is largely a cognitive event, it will only be considered here in the context of the outcome of the cognitive process. The outcome for this type of task is a judgment or decision. The information exchange process takes place through statements, both verbal and written. Statements can be classified as either transmitting information (sharing) or attempting to collect information (gathering). Sharing in the sense of role expansion is the act of transmitting information to others that exceeds the communication job requirements of the person transmitting the information. Gathering in the sense of role expansion is the collecting of information that is not directly part of a person's job. Sharing and gathering information is considered role expansion to the extent that these acts go beyond the requirements of the job and are job relevant. Both sharing or gathering information statements can be further divided into those that are either job related or non-job related. Job related statements include information that is useful to the decision task. A statement like "I need the current interest rate on 4 year certificates of deposit (CDs)" is an example of a job related information gathering statement in an investment decision making task. A statement like "You can only invest 4% of your gross wages" is an example of a job related information sharing statement in the same context. Non-job related statements include socializing comments or comments of encouragement or frustration. For example, a statement like, "don't worry about that last loan decision, most new loan officers make that same mistake, " is meant to console a team member and is not directly related to the task. These types of statements do not add 22 to the team's ability to make decisions and, according to Naylor, et al. (1980), would not be considered role relevant. Non-job related statements will not be considered role relevant in this study. Another form of job related statements would be information that is intended to develop a strategy among team members. These statements are job related because the purpose of the statement is to improve the team's decision making performance by coordinating team members' actions. For example, a statement like, "send me information on the interest rate of 4 year CDs every time you get updates" is a job related strategy forming statement. The purpose of this statement is to reduce the need to ask if there has been an update on interest. Finally, if a statement is job related then it can be in one of two forms, raw or translated. Raw information has not been translated into terms that can be understood by non-experts. No refinement or value judgments have been added to the original information. For example, a report that states that a company had $15 million in sales last year would be an example of raw information. From the information provided it would be difficult for a novice to evaluate what this sales figure means. However, if the report also stated that the company made $2.5 million in profit, and that this level of profit was outstanding for a firm of this size in this market, then a novice would now be able to understand the meaning of the information provided. These examples demonstrate that there are several ways that informational statements can be classified when considered from a role expansion perspective. In this study, all forms of communication that take place between team members will be added together to create a global measure of role expansion. Due to the lack of 23 theoretical work in the area of role expansion in decision making teams this uni- dimensional definition will be used when formulating hypotheses. In addition, written communication will be content coded using the coding scheme Shown in Figure 4. This will allow for an exploratory look into the possible multi-dimensionality of role expansion in decision making teams. Thus far, all of the theories discussed involve dyadic interactions, often between a leader or supervisor and a role incumbent. Because of the importance the leader/supervisor has in these theories, two leadership theories will be reviewed. The purpose is to identify characteristics of the leader that will effect the role expansion process. In addition, substitutes to those leader characteristics are examined. Leadership and Substitutes for Leaders Like the role literature, the leadership literature also has a long history of studying hierarchical dyadic relationships. There are many different theories of leadership and can be differentiated by their focus on the characteristics of the leader, the follower, and/or the situation. One theory of leadership comes from the Ohio State University studies and looks at two behavioral characteristics of the leader, consideration and initiating structure (Stogdill, 1974). Consideration is the degree to which a leader acts in a supportive manner and Shows concern for subordinates (Yukl, 1989). Examples of consideration are asking for ideas from subordinates and having a willingness to accept their suggestions. Initiating structure is the degree to which a leader defines and Structures his/her own role and the roles of subordinates (Yukl, 1989). Examples of initiating structure are offering new approaches to problems and coordinating the activities of subordinates. 24 Figure 4: Text message content coding scheme. / Task \ Social Strategy (1) (8) Seek or Provide Seey “Vida Role/Rule Role/Rule Target Target [72/ \Target / \rget TransmIt Transmit Non-trans R/R Non-trans. /\“’ /\m (3) MT) (6) (7) 25 Initially, it was thought that a good leader was a person who had rated high on both factors. However, Kerr and his associates (Kerr & Jermier, 1978; Howell, Dorfrnan & Kerr, 1986) have expanded on the original Ohio State University studies by showing that characteristics of the subordinate and/or situation can often substitute for consideration and initiating structure behaviors by the leader. For instance, a group that is highly cohesive could substitute for a leader that has a high rating on consideration. Group members would receive the same supportive behavior fiom the group as they would from the leader (Howell & Dorfman, 1981; Howell, Bowen, Dorfman, Kerr and Podsakoff, 1990). This is a situational factor that acts as a substitute for leadership. Another example is a subordinate who is highly professional and takes pride in his/her work and accomplishments (Childers, Dubinsky & Skinner, 1990). Here there would be little need for a supervisor who had a high rating on initiating structure. This is a subordinate factor that acts as a substitute for leadership. It is important to consider characteristics of the leader in conjunction with possible substitutes for leadership. There are two possible substitutes for leadership that seem most relevant to this discussion of role expansion. They are an incumbent's level of experience and the type of feedback that the task provides. Both Katz and Kahn (1978) and Naylor et al. (1980) recognized the importance of these two factors. Katz and Kahn suggested that a role incumbent's past experiences will affect how he/She responds to the sent role message. In addition, they theorized that an individual can send a role to him/herself. In this case the incumbent will use past experiences to help determine what role expectations will be sent. In developing their self role model, Naylor et al. (1980) also stated that a role incumbent's past experiences are 26 relevant to the role development process. Thus a role incumbent's past experiences can substitute for the leader's behaviors of consideration and initiating structure. To the extent that the past experiences are relevant to the current task, they may help the role incumbents structure their own tasks. In this case there would be little need for the role incumbent to depend on the leader to provide a supportive environment or to structure the task. Although experience has been tested as a moderator between leader characteristics and general satisfaction and organizational commitment (Howell & Dorfman, 1981, 1986; Childers, Dubinsky, & Skinner, 1990), role expansion has not been examined as the dependant variable. Another possible substitute for leadership is the type of feedback that is provided by the task. Part of what the leader or role sender does is Observe the behaviors or behavioral outcomes and provide the role incumbent with feedback. The feedback is then used by the role incumbent either to reinforce current behaviors or to modify them. Occasionally a task can provide this feedback directly. Past research has looked at task feedback as a moderator (Howell & Dorfman, 1981, 1986), however, this has not been done with role expansion as the criterion. Experiences and feedback provided by the tasks might also have a direct effect on role expansion. For these reasons the task experience and feedback literatures are reviewed in the following two sections. Research omTflErgoerience Experience is often used as a variable in organizational behavior research. Perhaps because of its wide use in common discourse, there has been little rigorous effort made to conceptualize and operationalize experience clearly as a construct. Often when 27 researchers talk about experience on the job, they operationalize it as the amount of time on the job (tenure). Schmidt, Hunter and Outerbridge (1986) did difi‘erentiate between organizational experience and job experience as the time in the organization and the time on a particular job within the organization. In both cases experience is operationalized as tenure. Experience has also been conceptualized as the number of different jobs and different departments within an organization where the incumbent has worked (Bons, Bass & Komorita, 1970; Alvares & Hulin, 1973; Burroughs, Rollins & Hopkins, 1973). This conceptualization of experience is based on the type of experience one has and not on tenure. An example is a worker who is rotated from one job to another as part of a program of job enrichment (Hackman & Oldham, 1975) or a manager who is rotated from department to department to gain experience in several firnctional areas within an organization (Burroughs et al., 1973). In these studies, the amount of time spent in any one job or department is generally not important. What is important is that the individual worked in several different jobs or departments. This kind of experience will be called multiple job experience. Experience has also been operationalized as the number of times a task is performed (Spiker, Harper, & Hayes, 1985). Experience was conceptualized in terms of the frequency of performing a task. This is referred to as task experience and is important because of another assumption that is often made when experience is operationalized as time. That is, more experience equals higher proficiency. However, if time is used to operationalize experience, the implication is that two individuals with the same tenure will have similar experiences (i.e., they will perform the 28 same set of tasks and they will perform the tasks the same number of times). It has been shown by Ford, Quinones, Sego and Sora (1992), Schmitt and Cohen (1989) and Vance, Coovert, MacCallun, and Hedge (1989) that this is not a tenable assumption. Ford et al. have shown that workers with the same tenure in the same job varied greatly on the tasks they performed as well as the number of times each task was performed. In this study, the focus will be on the tasks that are performed There are three ways to conceptualize experience: the amount of time on the job (tenure), the different types of positions held in the past (multiple job experience), and the frequency with which the task was performed (task experience). Multiple job experience seems most relevant as a substitute for leadership. The multiple job experience is important because this experience might lead a role incumbent to add to his/her current role. In short, the greater the diversity in experiences, the greater the number of role elements that are likely to be relevant and thus included in the incumbent's role. Research on Feedbagk Feedback plays a very important role in many areas in organizational behavior and industrial psychology. Feedback is found in goal setting (Locke & Latham 1990), control theory (Carver & Scheier, 1982; Campion & Lord, 1982), performance appraisal (Pearce & Porter, 1986; Greller & Parsons, 1992) and learning (Schmitt, Coyle, & King, 1976). A review by Ilgen, Fisher, & Taylor (1979) has shown the importance of when feedback is given, whether the feedback is positive or negative, and the importance of the credibility of the feedback source. Although there has been a great deal of research on feedback, most of the studies reviewed by Ilgen et al. dealt with outcome feedback. Outcome feedback is defined as information on the overall result of performance. 29 An example of outcome feedback is a supervisor telling a worker that the last product he/she produced did not meet quality standards. Outcome feedback does not provide information Specific enough to isolate problems. More specific information is critical if feedback is going to be useful in making changes that will lead to improved performance. Recently a new line of inquiry into feedback has examined cognitive feedback (Balzer, Doherty & O'Conner, 1989). Cognitive feedback refers to information about relations rather than outcomes (Todd & Hammond, 1965). In a decision making task, this might mean feedback on how different pieces of information are related, rather than information about the correctness or accuracy of the decision. There are three types of cognitive feedback: task information, cognitive information, and functional validity information (Balzer et al. (1989). Task information is the most relevant in developing a model of role expansion. Within the theory of cognitive feedback, task information: refers to relations between the cues and the criterion (Ye), information about the criterion or the cues themselves, or both. (Balzer et al., 1989, p. 412) The following example clarifies this definition and illustrates the difference between outcome and task information feedback. A bank manager tells a loan office employee only that her last loan decision was bad for the bank. This is an example of outcome feedback. If, instead, the bank manager had told the loan officer that her loan decision was bad because the person seeking the loan did not have enough collateral for 30 the amount requested in the loan, then this would be an example of task information feedback. The bank manager connected task information, the amount Of collateral, with the criterion, whether or not to grant the loan request. The discussion about role expansion and the factors effecting role expansion include several references to tasks, task experience, task information and task elements. To avoid any firrther confusion I will use the above definition of task information within the theory of cognitive feedback but will refer to it as process feedback. Theoretical Model The model developed is created to explain how leaders, individuals and tasks can influence the role expansion process within decision making teams where incumbents have individual areas of expertise and are interdependent. This model tries to explain how individual and task or situational factors contribute to the role expansion process. The model makes the following assumptions. First, the model assumes that the task is one that requires some interdependence with other people. This is because the two main categories of emergent role elements in a complex decision making task are gathering and sharing of information. A decision whether or not to close a factory is an example of a complex decision task. This decision requires input fiom many people including the organization's legal staff, its manufacturing department, labor unions and the local government. Though each group is expert in its particular field, it can supply only a small part of the total information needed when considering a factory closing option. Consequently, interdependency results from the situation where no one individual or group could possess all the information and expertise required to make a complex decision. 31 Second, the model assumes that the acts of gathering and sharing specific pieces of information are examples of role elements. This is true as long as the information gathered or shared is relevant to the role. Finally, the model assumes that whether roles expand or not, the criteria for evaluating performance remains the same. This is an important point because frequently as a role changes, so do the standards for evaluating that role. However, this is not necessarily the casein decision making tasks. The evaluation criterion remains the team member's decision making accuracy. That is, individual team members are evaluated on their contribution to the overall team decision. Thus, in decision making teams with distributed expertise the team members are expected to process the information on which they are the expert and make their recommendation based on that information. Regardless of whether a team member expands his/her role to include other pieces of information or not, the evaluation of his/her performance is still based on the quality of the judgment he/She makes. Model of Role Expansion The role expansion model presented here de-emphasizes the dyadic negotiation in the role expansion process. Instead this model allows for situational factors and characteristics of the role incumbent to influence the role expansion process. Based on the substitute for leadership literature it is also posited that the incumbent and situational factors will also moderate the effect of the leader's behavior and role expansion. The role expansion model is presented in Figure 5. Unlike much of the research on roles that focuses on outcome variables such as role ambiguity and role conflict (Caplan & Jones, 197 5; Dailey, Ickinger & Coote, 1986), the central purpose of this model is to 32 Figure 5: Role expansion model. Incumbent Characteristics Expenence Leader Characteristics Consideration Initiating Struct. Situational Characteristics Feedback Role Expansion 33 describe the factors that affect the role expansion process. This model tries to explain why some role incumbents take on emergent role elements, thus expanding their roles while others limit their roles to correspond more closely to the original job description. The role expansion model attempts to build on VDL research (Graen, Orris, & Johnson, 1973; Graen, Novak & Sommerkamp, 1982; Wakabayashi and Graen, 1984) by looking beyond the leader incumbent dyad to include Situational factors. In addition, this model leaves open the possibility that some Situational or incumbent factors might substitute for the leader and consequently decrease the importance of the dyadic relationship in certain situations. The model also suggests that there are outcome consequences to the role expansion process. Performance is posited as one outcome consequence of the role expansion process. Performance is affected either positively or negatively by role expansion depending on certain characteristics of the role incumbent and the task. The role expansion to performance relationship is of secondary importance in this study. The primary purpose of this study is to understand what factors effect role expansion behaviors by the role incumbent. The starting point of the role expansion model is the leader. Past research has been mixed in determining the effects of initiating structure and consideration on such outcomes as work group productivity, incumbent satisfaction, and turnover (see Stogdill, 1974). Despite this, one could make several predictions about the relationship between these behaviors and incumbent role expansion. When the leader's level of consideration is high, this should lead to greater participation in the negotiating process because the role incumbent is receiving 34 encouragement and is being listened to by the leader. In general, it is predicted that greater role expansion will occur when the leader's level of consideration is high than when the leader's level of consideration is low. This prediction lead to the following hypothesis: Hyp 1: Higher consideration by the leader will result in greater role expansion by the team members. The effects of initiating structure will be quite different. When a leader's level of initiating structure is low, there will be a greater amount of both sharing and collecting of information in all categories by team members. This is similar to the case when consideration is high. There will be less sharing and collecting of information in general then when a leader's level of initiating structure is high. High levels of initiating structure by leaders will create a more autocratic environment resulting in less information sharing and gathering opportunities. This prediction lead to the following hypothesis: Hyp 2: Lower initiating structure by the leader will result in greater role expansion by the team members. There are two other sources that are predicted to influence the role expansion process of team members. They are divided into two categories as shown in Figure 5. One category includes characteristics of the incumbent while the other category includes characteristics of the task. Experience is the incumbent characteristic while the type of 35 task feedback is the task characteristic. Experience is considered high when the role incumbent has held a variety of job positions that are in the current task. If a role incumbent has only experienced one role position then he/she will not be likely to expand his/her own role without influence from the leader. If, however, the role incumbent has experienced several relevant role positions in the past, then he/she will be more likely to incorporate some of those past role elements into the current role. It is predicted that high experience will in general be related to greater role expansion. Role incumbents are likely to attempt to incorporate their past experiences into their role. This will lead to increases in all forms of information gathering and sharing behaviors. These predictions lead to the following hypothesis: Hyp 3: Higher amounts of past experiences on the part of the role incumbent will be associated with greater role expansion. The substitute for leadership literature suggests that there are times when characteristics of the task or the incumbent make the leader's behavior irrelevant. For example, when the role incumbent is high on past experience it is predicted that this will make the leader's standing on consideration irrelevant. Past experience will act as a substitute for consideration. When experience is high, it does not matter whether the leader's level Of consideration is low or high. This relationship is shown in Figure 6 and leads to the following hypothesis: 36 Figure 6: The moderating effects of experience on the leader's behavior of consideration to incumbent role expansion relationship. .c 2" I O o c 8 t o 8 \ Vic-2 S ._ \ IDXO z ‘50.; \ 5'5“ (U .9 \ v.8 adj]: \\ 85 \ 48 \ 0 \ 3 \ O _I .c .9 8 I ...l c .9 m 8 %e EU] 37 Hyp 4: Consideration and task experience will interact to affect role expansion. The nature of this interaction is such that the relationship between the leader behavior of consideration and role expansion is only negative when experience is low. If experience is high it substitutes for the leader's behavior, so that role expansion occurs regardless of the level of consideration. Another prediction has to do with the interaction between experience and initiating structure. Once again experience is predicted to act as a substitute for a leader's behavior (initiating structure). When there is a high amount of past experience it will make the leader's standing on initiating structure irrelevant. The nature of the interaction is Shown in Figure 7. The prediction leads to the following hypothesis: Hyp 5: Initiating structure and task experience will interact to affect role expansion. The nature of this interaction is such that the relationship between the leader's behavior of initiating structure and role expansion is only positive when experience is high. If experience is low it substitutes for the leader behavior, so that role expansion is limited regardless of the level of initiating structure. A characteristic of the task that is of interest in this study is the type of feedback the role incumbent receives. As mentioned before, the two types of feedback are outcome and process feedback. Outcome feedback tells the role incumbent whether or not his/her 38 Figure 7: The moderating effects of experience on the leader's behavior of initiating structure to incumbent role expansion relationship. High Expefience High Past InItIatIng Structure Past Experience Low Low High Low Role Expansion 39 decision was correct. If it was incorrect, then the direction and magnitude of the error is also contained in the feedback information. Process feedback gives the role incumbent information about the specific pieces of information used in the decision task as well as the relationship between these pieces of information. Process feedback should lead to increased proficiency on the task. As proficiency increases, incumbents Should attempt to expand their roles. Consequently the following prediction is made: Hyp 6: Role incumbent's receiving process feedback will be associated with greater role expansion. Process feedback should also act as a substitute for the leader's behavior. The leader's standing on consideration and initiating structure is made irrelevant when the role incumbent is receiving process feedback from the task. The nature of the interaction is shown in Figures 8 and 9 and in the following two predictions: Hyp 7: Consideration and feedback will interact to effect role expansion. The nature of this interaction is such that the relationship between the leader's level of consideration and role expansion is only negative for role incumbents receiving outcome feedback. If role incumbents only received process feedback it substitutes for the leader behavior, so that role expansion occurs regardless of the level of consideration. Hyp 8: Initiating structure and feedback will interact to affect role 40 Figure 8: The moderating effects of feedback on the leader's behavior of consideration to incumbent role expansion relationship. .C .9 \ I \ \\ $36 \ Em \ o-O \ 0'0 C x \ 5e .9 (no \ 0 e-- a)“, \OLI. he 0% \ m0 8o \ 13:9 ho \ (Um O.I.L \ ‘1’: \ 48 \ \ \ \ \ \\ 3 \ O _l .c 3 .9 o I .J C .9 (D 8 -go. mil 41 Figure 9: The moderating effects of feedback on the leader's behavior of initiating structure to incumbent role expansion relationship. High Process Feedback Outcome Feed back InItIatIng Structure Low High Low Role Expansion 42 expansion. The nature of this interaction is such that the relationship between the leader behavior of initiating structure and role expansion is only positive when role incumbents receive process feedback. When role incumbents receive outcome feedback it substitutes for the leader behavior, so that role expansion is limited regardless of the level of initiating structure. Understanding the factors that affect role expansion is important and is the primary purpose of this study. It is also important to understand the link between role expansion and performance. For example, if an incumbent's role is expanded and his/her performance is poor, then the organization is likely to see role expansion as negative. On the other hand, if performance is good, then role expansion will be seen as positive. The second part the of the study examines the consequence of role expansion on decision making performance. This study looks at two factors that are hypothesized to interact with role expansion to effect the role incumbent's decision making performance. The first, task knowledge, is a characteristic of the incumbent. The second, the stress involved in performing the task, is a characteristic of the situation. These are shown in Figure 10. In this study, task knowledge is firndamentally important to performance. Sharing and gathering information will not lead to high performance unless the incumbent has the task knowledge necessary to use the information. This is especially true when the information that is being received is in an uninterpreted form. In this case the nature of 43 Figure 10: Incumbent and task characteristics as moderators of the role expansion to task performance relationship. Incumbent Characteristics Task Knowledge 1 fl Task Performance Role Expansion Stress Task Characteristics 44 the interaction between role expansion and task knowledge is described in Figure 11 and the following hypothesis: Hyp 9: Role expansion and task knowledge will interact to effect decision making performance. The nature of this interaction is such that the relationship between role expansion and decision making performance is highest when task knowledge is high. This relationship is lowest when task knowledge is low and role expansion is high. There are several characteristics of the task that can have an effect on performance. One of these is the amount of stress that is involved in performing the task. Stress can be created from several sources including time constraints, the physical environment where the task is performed and physical demands of the tasks (Ivancevich & Matteson, 1980). One way to lessen the stress iS to share some of the work load with others. This is what happens in hierarchical decision making teams with distributed expertise. This is not to say that in these teams time constraints cannot create Stress, but only that the nature of these types of teams help to reduce the effect of the stress. Role expansion, however, could have a negative impact on team decision making performance. If, over time, each team member takes on an increasing amount of the work load in the form of emergent role elements, this would produce a role that is too large, complex and difficult to execute under conditions of high stress. This effect is shown in Figure 12 and lead to the following hypothesis: 45 Figure l 1: The moderating effects of task knowledge on the role expansion to task performance relationship. 0 g) 8: 33 3 3 ‘3‘ 2 2 X s: 21.: x a? 88 i—I I—_I .C I .9? , :I: / / / / C , .9 (D / s / %O_ ,’ mil / / I / 3 / O .J ‘1: .9 8 I _J Performance Task 46 Figure 12: The moderating effects of stress on the role expansion to task performance relationship. VI m m 8 .c 9 3 u 9 *" 0 a I m _I [ .C x .9 \ :I: \ \ \ C ‘ .9 \ 2 \ (I) w ‘ '5 3 \ tr LLI \ \ \ \ \ 3 \ _l .C .9 E :I: .1 Performance Task 47 Hyp 10: Role expansion and stress will interact to affect decision making performance. The nature of this interaction is such that the relationship between role expansion and decision making performance will be highest when role expansion is high and stress is low. When role expansion is high and stress is high the result will be the lowest decision making performance. Summa_ry The primary focus of this study is to examine the antecedents of role expansion with the secondary focus being on decision making accuracy as a consequence of role expansion. Based on the theoretical work by Ilgen and Hollenbeck (1991) it was predicted that the subjects in this study would add role elements to the jobs they were assigned. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that role expansion would be predictable based on characteristics of the leader, the situation, the individual, and their interactions. Finally, it was hypothesized that role expansion would interact with job knowledge and stress to effect decision making accuracy. The first part of the study deals with four factors that are predicted to effect role expansion. First, it is posited that leaders that display high amounts of consideration or low amounts of initiating structure behaviors will lead to increased role expansion by subordinate team members. Second, it is posited that incumbents who experience more than one job within the team will be more likely to expand their roles than incumbents who experience only one job within the team. Third, it is posited that incumbents who receive process feedback (task level cognitive feedback) will be more likely to expand their role than incumbents who received only outcome feedback. Fourth, it is posited that 48 experience and feedback will act as substitutes for the leader's behavior of consideration and initiating structure. The second part of the study deals with the three factors that are predicted to effect decision making accuracy. It is posited that under high stress conditions, role expansion will lead to poorer performance, while under low stress conditions it will lead to higher performance. Also it is posited that an incumbent with high job knowledge and high role expansion will make the most accurate decisions and an incumbent with low job knowledge and high role expansion will make the least accurate decisions. Chapter Three: Method Section Subjects The subjects used in this study were students in an introductory management class at a large Midwestern university. All students in the class were given an opportunity to participate by signing up for a single, three-hour time slot. When they arrived at the Team Effectiveness Research Lab, four subjects were randomly selected to participate in the experiment. The remaining subjects, if any, were directed to another experiment that was running concurrently in the lab. Each four-person team was then randomly assigned to one of four experimental conditions. Each team participated in a Team Interactive Decision Exercise for Teams Incorporating Distributed Expertise (TIDEZ) computer Simulation (Hollenbeck, Sego, Ilgen, & Major, l99l). TIDE2 Simulation The TIDE2 simulation was set up for a naval command and control center configuration. The simulation program parameters were set up for a four person team that had access to a total of nine information cues (target attributes). The four stations included an aircraft carrier (the team decision maker and leader), a coastal air defense (CAD) station, an advance warning airborne command system (AWACS) aircraft, and a naval cruiser. The last three stations will be referred to as the outer stations and these are the jobs that will be examined for role expansion in this study. The carrier will be referred to as the leader. The Object of the simulation was to determine the level of threat that an unidentified aircraft represented to the team. Each team member gathered information about the target that was thought to be necessary for making a decision. Specifically, the 49 50 outer stations made recommendations or judgments to the team leader, who in turn made the overall team decision. Figures 13 and 14 Show the parameter settings for the TIDE2 Simulation. These are fiIrther described below. All stations had access to three pieces of information (cues) about the target. The cues had the following characteristics. First, there were three levels for each cue, non- threatening, somewhat threatening, and very threatening. A cue that was non-threatening was assigned a value of zero. A somewhat threatening cue was assigned a value of one. A very threatening cue was assigned a value of two. Second, each cue was weighted for its main effect. All cues had a main effect of zero except for Identification Friend or Foe (IFF) which had a cue weight of two. Thus, IFF could range from zero to four (i.e., if the cue value was two the cue weight was two, 2 x 2 = 4). The TIDE2 simulation allows for up to four interactions. In this study there were four two-way interactions. Each interaction had a weight of one. This allows the interactions also to take on values of zero, one, two, or four. As a result the one main effect and four two-way interactions were weighted equally. Each outer station had access to at least one interaction. The team leader had access to the one main effect cue. This left one interaction that was not directly accessible by any one outer station. This created a Situation where two stations had to communicate with each other in order to gather and share information necessary to determine the value of the fourth interaction. The Simulation created two characteristics that were important for this study. The first is that there was distributed expertise among team members. Each outer station had exclusive access to information and knowledge to translate the raw information into the 51 Figure 13: Weights and values settings for the TIDE2 simulation (screen 1). mmvoz mufluz OO 3 HO .mwmcmno Hmucmo Ou O .omconu Ou H**H +5500 How m .+mwuzmcmz ROM < .meOH >mapoz\3ofl> Ou H "umucm Om Om me OH HmHHzxzxzxzxz H H H H HmHHuemHmz "mcoHeumRmucH OO O OO om OON OOH E O emcee O O O O O O m H mmmHO H emcee O O OH Hm ON HH OH O use HE H maneum Reoo O O OH O Om HH om Hm Ode H eoHeomuHo O O O.H m.H m.H O. O. m. an: H mmH m O O. OH- O O. OH O Owe m eHmca O O OH OH OO Hm me He E m eNHm O m OOOOH OOOO OOOmm HOOmH ooomm HOOmm OH H epsuHuHc O m OOO Hmm Omm HOO OOH OOH eds H cemdm O H IIIIIII HHHOIIIIII- HOHH HOB HO_ HOE * m u OEHO uflm3 wmcmmmz Hog q n mGOOH # mm. m u moEOouso * HvH m n mucmempsh * Hma e u mCOHuOonudH # Hm“ m mousmmoz * HHH monam> a munmflwz 603.mem >MHUOZ 52 Figure 14: Weights and values settings for the TIDE2 simulation (screen 2). Omvoz OOOHS O» 3 HO .mmmcmno Hoodoo Ou O .mmcmnu Ou Htwa +8500 How m .+mwuzmmwz How d .mcooH %MOCOZ\BOO> O» H "Hmucm O w w > "one on cue HONH w w O O O O w w w O O O H9H. axes one HON. » O O O "moo 0» cue HONL O O O w w w O O w w w 9 ”9H. msHm> one HONH O m m H O w w w w w w w w w w 9 H9O Russo Hmma m m H O m H O m H O m m "OE sonb HONIONH new mom O O O O m m m m m m m m H H H H HEoum HHana 4 O m m H a O m m H < O m m H < O m m H O0 Hoebpmmm m>ocm HNNL Rom Hem O O O O O m m m m m m m m m m H H H H H HHccHERmE z w z z z z z w z z z z z w z w w w w w "xocncemm 3mH> Hmmm O "O m\o HHNO m "Hm\o\mv some xoeppomm HON_ m- mmemmmHo HI OzmoHOzH ON pseudo O HouHeoz Omm ummHsuu O mmHz OH eonooo O 3mH>wm OOO 0<3< H OOH: mmmz OH Somme m eeocOH OmH nae N BHm HO CAMS o HOMO Oz mmm HOOHHOU HOH_ .OHL HOHH HOH_ HOHH mOSHm> O munmflmz BO3.OQXO %MOCOZ 53 appropriate threat level. Second, there was interdependence, in that members had to share both information and expertise. There are seven possible responses to each target in this configuration of TIDE2 used in this study (ignore, review, monitor, warn, ready, lock- on, or defend). Each response has a target value associated with it, as shown in Figure 15. Thus, there is a true score or correct decision for each target. The outer stations' judgments are compared with the correct decision to determine their judgment accuracy. Research desiga In this study there were seven independent variables and two dependent variables. The independent variables included: leader consideration, leader initiating structure, job knowledge, experience, feedback, stress, and role expansion. The dependent variables included: role expansion and decision making performance. Note that role expansion was used as both an independent and dependent variable. Role expansion was the dependent variable in the first analysis and an independent variable in the second analysis. This study was divided into two parts. The first part of the study had four independent variables, two of which were manipulated and two of which were measured. The two manipulated variables in this experiment were experience and feedback. Subjects in the "low experience" condition remained in a single job for the entire experiment. Subjects in the "high experience" condition were rotated between all three of the outer station jobs during the experiment. There were two types of performance information created by the feedback manipulation. Subjects in the "outcome feedback" condition received feedback on how accurate their judgments were compared with the correct judgment. In addition, they were aware of the direction of their error (i.e., whether they were over aggressive or 54 under aggressive). Subjects in the "process feedback" condition received feedback on each cue and interaction that they could access. This was in addition to outcome feedback. The two independent variables that were measured but not manipulated were the leader's behavioral characteristics of consideration and initiating structure. The dependant variable in the first part of the study was the amount of role expansion that took place for each outer station team member. The second part of the study had three independent variables, one of which was manipulated and two of which were measured. The manipulated variable was stress. Targets in the last 16 trials were randomly assigned to either a "high" or "low" stress condition. In the high stress condition subjects were given only 90 seconds to make their judgments about the targets. In the low stress condition subjects were given 160 seconds to make their judgments about the targets. The two independent variables that were measured but not manipulated were job knowledge and role expansion. Job knowledge was a measure of how well a subject understood the information that made up their specific job. Role expansion was a measure of the number of role elements that each subject added to their in the first part of the study. The dependent variable in the second part of the experiment was the decision making accuracy of each station. Manipulations amd measures Experience. High experience was operationalized by rotating all outer station team members in this condition though all of the outer station positions. In the low experience condition the subjects worked at only one of the outer stations (CAD, 55 AWACS, or Cruiser). At the beginning of the experiment each subject was trained on the job Specifics of one station in the high experience condition. Subjects had two practice trials followed by six more trials. At the end of the eighth trial the outer station subjects were given a new assignment (different computer station) followed by another Six trials. The subjects were again rotated to new assignment after the 14th trial. In the low experience condition subjects were assigned to one station and remained there for the entire experiment. Feedback. The feedback provided by the task was one of two types. The first was outcome feedback and consisted of the feedback on the accuracy of each station's decision on each trial. This allowed each team member to determine the accuracy of his/her judgment. In cases where the judgment was incorrect, the feedback also provided the direction of the error. That is, whether the judgment was over aggressive or under aggressive. The second type of feedback was process feedback. In the process feedback condition each team member received the same performance information for each trial as that provided in the outcome feedback condition. In addition, the process feedback condition supplied each team member with specific feedback on each information cue on which he/she had experience. This included a representation of the raw data, the correct translation of each piece of data and, in the case of interactions, the correct translation of the interaction. For example, if the speed of the target was 600 mph, then the process feedback repeated the raw value of the information (600mph) and the translated value, which, in this case, is very threatening. Because speed interacts with direction, the result of the interaction is also provided in the process feedback condition. In one example, the 56 direction was 5 degrees, which was also very threatening and the interaction between speed and direction was thus very dangerous. Figures 15 through 18 show the process feedback that each station received after the first practice trial. Note that although two stations had access to information on speed, only one station had access to information on both speed and direction. The two feedback conditions thus vary on both the level of specificity and in the shear amount of information provided. The two feedback conditions receive the same outcome feedback. In addition the process feedback condition receives information about each cue that can be accessed directly. This information is more Specific than the outcome feedback information. It could be argued that the feedback manipulation results in two conditions with one condition simply receiving more feedback information than the other. However, in this case, the nature of the added feedback is more important than the volume. In the process feedback condition subjects receive information that should help them understand where they made errors in interpreting the individual importance of each cue. This should lead to greater learning of each cues and should result in subjects in this condition out performing subjects in the outcome feedback condition. Process feedback naturally provides a greater amount of information because it provides more detail. However, it is the specificity of information and not the amount of information that is critical to role expansion. Outcome feedback does not help isolate the cause of errors in decision making. It only informs the decision maker that an error has occurred. Decision making performance improvements would largely be a result of trial and error learning under this condition, but this is difficult given the complexity of the task in terms of the cues to criterion relationships. Process feedback, however, does provide 57 Figure 15: Process feedback form for the Carrier position. Altitude = 26,367 ft ----- non-threatening IFF = .5 Mhz ------------- non-threatening Radar = Class 9 ---------- very threatening 58 Figure 16: Process feedback form for the CAD position. Speed = 325 mph ---------- non-threatening ----- non-dangerous Direction = 27 dgs ------- non-threatening Size = 27 meters --------- very threatening 59 Figure 17: Process feedback form for the AWACS position. Angle = +15 dgs ------- very threatening ----- non-dangerous Range = 200 miles ----- non-threatening Radar = Class 9 ------- very threatening 60 Figure 18: Process feedback form for the Cruiser position. Altitude = 26,367 ft ----- non-threatening ----- non-dangerous Corridor Status = 5 mi --- non-threatening Size = 27 meters --------- very threatening 61 information that is specific enough to isolate the errors in decision making. It thus allows for greater Speed in taking corrective action that will lead to improved decision making performance. The method used to supply the process feedback to subjects after each trial was as follows: at the end of each trial the team's feedback summary appeared on the screen. The summary screen contained outcome feedback. A number varying from 1 to 99 appeared at the bottom of the screen next to the word "CODE". This number referred to a specific page in the feedback manual that each subject had at the computer stations. By matching the number on the computer screen with the correct page in the feedback manual, subjects received the process feedback for the trial they just completed. Although the subjects had all of the feedback sheets available for viewing during the simulation, it was impossible to use the feedback books to cheat. The order of the feedback sheets was randomly determined and a large number of false feedback sheets were included. Indeed, there were two pages of false feedback for every one page of valid feedback. Any attempt to accelerate learning or enhance performance by looking at the information in the feedback manuals was a waste of time. This was explained to the subjects at the beginning of the experiment. A review of the text messages suggests that only one team attempted to use the feedback manuals to help them make their decisions. Further analysis showed that this did not help or greatly hurt their performance and after six trials the team abandoned this Strategy. Lgader characteristics. Characteristics of the leader were measured by content coding the text messages for each leader/outer station dyad. This allowed for the leader to be coded independently with respect to each member of the team. The leaders were rated 62 on two leader behavior characteristics, consideration and initiating structure. The scale used and the instruction given to those coding the text messages are shown in Appendix A. Consideration and initiating structure were examined at the dyadic level. That is, for each leader/subordinate pair (there were three pairs for each team) a separate set of ratings were made. Two raters who were blind to the purpose of the experiment were used to code all 240 leader/subordinate pairs. The inter-rater reliability was .88 for consideration and .92 for initiating structure as shown in Table 1. This method was chosen over using the leader behavior description questionnaire (LBDQ) for two reasons. Fiist, most of the items of the leader behavior description questionnaire are not relevant to the types of interactions that took place while participating in the TIDE2 simulation. For example, the following is an items from the LBDQ, "He does personal favors for group members" (Schriesheim & Stogdill, 1975). In the TIDE2 simulation there were no opportunities to display a behavior like the one described in the above LBDQ item. Second, by content coding the text messages, each leader/subordinate pair could be coded. This also meant that the resulting ratings were not self reports, but were the judgements of two independent raters. Role Expamsion. The primary dependant variable for the first part of the study was role expansion. Role expansion refers to emergent role elements that are added to a team member's job. Each subject was given a specific job at the beginning of the experiment. These jobs require that each outer station member measure three attributes of the unknown target. Two of the three attributes interact with each other and it is the responsibility of each outer station member to measure and interpret this information. The 63 Table l: Inter-rater reliability for the leader consideration and initiating structure scales and text message coding. Number of Scale Alpha Raters Consideration .88 2 Initiating Structure .92 2 Text messages .94 2 64 final interaction required the CAD to transmit size to the AWACS. Finally, each outer station was required to make a recommendation to the team leader on how to respond to each target. This constitutes the job of each outer station. There were no additional requirement of the job for team members to communicate with one another, although the ability to communication with one another was available to each team member. Role expansion occurred when a team member engaged in behaviors that are not explicitly defined as part of their job. For example, if a team member Shared information with another team member this would be considered role expansion because it goes beyond that member's job description. Requesting information from other team members would also be considered role expansion. In Short, any Sharing of information or any type of communication not demanded by the formal job description was considered role expansion in this study. The dimensionality of role expansion was tested by conducting an exploratory factor analysis of communication messages. No a priori factors were predicted because there is currently no literature to support such predictions. The first step was to code all text messages using the coding scheme shown in Figure 4. This coding scheme resulted in eight categories of communication. The text message categories are: 1) social communication, 2) requests for information about someone else's job, such as the range and boundaries of the information a team member can measure, 3) requests for information that could be transmitted by an alternative method, 4) requests for information that could not be transmitted by an alternative method, 5) the sharing with others information about one's job, such as the range and boundaries of information that can be measured, 6) the sharing with others information 65 that could be transmitted by an alternative method, 7) the sharing with others information that could not be transmitted by an alternative method, and 8) communication used in an attempt to develop a strategy for use during the simulation. Three people were used to code these messages. Two raters coded the entire set of communications while the third rater coded one fifth of the communications. The inter- rater reliability for the coding of the text messages into categories was .94 (see Table 1). The results from the factor analysis revealed a two factor solution that included five items in the first factor and four in the second. Social communications did not adequately load on either factor. The coefficient alphas were .57 for the first factors and .37 for the second. Because of the low reliability on these two factors an additional factor analysis was conducted which forced a single factor solution. The reliability of this factor was an alpha of .53. Given the low reliability of all factor solutions, i made a decision to choose the single factor solution for two reasons. First, all of the hypotheses are based on a uni-dimensional definition of the role expansion construct. Without compelling statistical evidence of a more complex multi—dimensional structure, the more parsimonious single factor of role expansion dimension was used to test the hypotheses. Second, the single factor solution had only a Slightly lower reliability than did the first factor in the two factor solution. Although neither reliability is particularly good, the lack of substantial difference makes the Single factor a viable option. The TIDE2 computer simulation task consisted of 36 trials. Role expansion was measured by averaging the number of emergent task elements for each team member during trials 3-20. 66 _S_tg§§. Stress was manipulated in the last 16 trials of the experiment. The stress manipulation was accomplished by varying the amount of time available to the subjects for making a decision. The high stress condition included trials with only 90 seconds per target. The low stress condition included trials with 160 seconds per target. High and low stress trials were randomly distributed. Role incumbent knowledga. The subjects' knowledge of the information cues and their ability to interpret the raw information that made up their jobs was assessed with a test. The test was given at the beginning of the simulation to measure each subject's task knowledge. The internal consistency for the job knowledge test was determined by calculating the coefficient alpha. Table 2 Shows that the coefficient alpha for the job knowledge test was an acceptable .75 (Nunnally, 1978). :1‘_e_am decision making accuracy. The second dependent variable was decision making accuracy. Decision making accuracy was the absolute difference between the judgment of each subject and the correct decision for each target. The direction of the error was not critical to this study. This measure was automatically recorded at the end of each trial as part of the data that the TIDE2 simulation collected. Procedures. An overview of the timing of the procedures is shown in Table 3. The subjects began the experiment by reading a handbook that described the simulation, the nine target attributes, and their Specific roles in their teams (see Appendix B). This was followed by the first administration of the task knowledge test (see Appendix C). At this point the subjects began the computer simulation with a short training session that introduced the subjects to the computer simulation environment (see Appendix D). In the high experience condition subjects rotated every 6 trials. The next 16 trials were used to 67 Table 2: Internal consistency for the task knowledge test across all four conditions. Number of Station Alpha Items Q Total .75 15 CAD .79 1 5 AWACS .69 1 5 Cruiser .77 1 5 68 Table 3: The timing and order of procedures used in the experiment. 1. Training session a. Review task description document b. View video (5 minute version) c. Task knowledge test d Interactive training 2. First 20 trials a. Teams in the high experience condition changed positions every 6 trials. They were given 8 minutes to study their new roles. b. All teams were allowed to communicate among team members during the 8 minutes. The leaders were told that they could use this time any way they wanted to including discussing Strategy. Outer station members were not instructed as to what to communicate only that the time was available. 3. Next 16 trials a. Each team member remained at the same station they were at when finishing the previous section. During this portion of the experiment Stress was manipulated by varying the time for each trial from 90 to 160 seconds. 4. Post session a. Team members filled out a second task knowledge test. b. Subjects were debriefed as to the purpose of the experiment and dismissed. 69 filled out a post-experiment questionnaire that included the second task knowledge test and role expansion measure. Before the subjects left, they were given a debriefing sheet (see Appendix E). Any questions the subjects had were answered at that time. Data Analysis Prima_ry data analysis. This study is divided into two parts. The purpose of the first part was to determine the antecedents to role expansion. Hierarchical regression analysis was used to test Hypotheses 1 through 8 as shown in Table 4. The first step included characteristics of the leader, testing the influence of the leader's behavior on role expansion. The second step included characteristics of the Situation, testing the influence of the type of feedback and experience on role expansion. The third step included the interactions between leader behavior and the situational factors, testing the substitute for leader behavior hypotheses. Secondary dat_a analysis. The purpose of the second part of the study was to look at the effects of role expansion, Stress and job knowledge on decision making performance. Hierarchical regression analysis was used again, this time to test hypotheses 9 and 10 as shown in Table 5. The first step included role expansion which was an independent variable in the second part of the study. The second step included job knowledge, stress and their interaction. The third step included the interactions of role expansion with job knowledge and stress testing hypotheses 9 and 10. Power Analysis. As recommended by Cohen and Cohen (1983), a power analysis was conducted to determine the sample size required to achieve the appropriate power level for the statistical analyses used to test the hypotheses presented in the previous chapter. The analysis requiring the greatest statistical power is that which tests the 70 Table 4: Data analysis plan for the regression equation where role expansion is the dependent variable. Step Independent Variable Hypothesis Tested 1 Consideration (C) 1 Initiating Structure (IS) C x IS - 2 Experience (EXP) 3 Type of Feedback (FDBK) 4 EXP x FDBK - 3 C x EXP 5 C x FDBK 6 IS x EXP 7 IS x F DBK 8 71 Table 5: Data analysis plan for the regression equation where team decision making performance is the dependent variable. Step Independent firiable Hypothesis Tested 1 Role Expansion (RE) - 2 Stress - Job Knowledge (JK) - STR_JK - 3 RE x JK 9 RE x Stress 10 72 hypotheses suggesting moderated effects. Ifthe entire regression equation explains at least 16% of the variance (R2 = .16), 84 teams would provide a power of .80 at the .05 one tail test level for a regression equation with seven variables. Although this study focused on individuals within a team, the experimental design will allow for fiiture analysis at the team level. As a result, the power analysis was calculated to determine the number of teams that would be required. This results in a sample size that should be more than adequate for the analyses at the individual level. The final number of teams that participated in the study was 80. During the data collection period there were 96 teams scheduled. One team's data was lost when the simulation experienced technical difficulties and the simulation was improperly restarted. 15 other teams were not run due to the lack of subjects. Six subjects were recruited for each team. However, for any number of reasons, including sickness, mid-term examinations, or social commitments, three or fewer subjects often showed up. Because a minimum of four subjects are required for the simulation, those who showed up were either rescheduled for another time or asked to participate in another study. Chapter Four: Results The means and standard deviations for all variables are presented in Table 6 and the intercorrelations among the variables are presented in Table 7. Moderator regression analysis was used to test hypotheses in this study. The first regression equation tested all hypotheses relating to role expansion. The second regression equation tested all hypotheses relating to decision making accuracy. Analysis of the Role Expansion Hypotheses Hypothesis 1. Hypothesis I predicted a main effect for the leader's level of consideration on role expansion. The prediction was that the higher leaders were rated on consideration the greater the amount of role expansion. Table 8 Shows the results of the moderator regression analyses used to test this hypothesis. Consideration accounted for 6.1% of the variance role expansion (R2 = .061, p < .01). Figure 19 Shows that the effect was in the predicted direction, the more consideration by the leader resulted in greater amounts of role expansion by the subordinate. Hypothesis 2. Hypothesis 2 predicted a main effect for the leader's level of initiating structure on role expansion. The specific prediction made by Hypothesis 2 was that when leaders were rated low on initiating structure there would be greater role expansion. Initiating structure accounted for only .4% of the variance (R2 = .004, not significant (ns) at the p < .5). The interaction between the leader's level of consideration and initiating structure was also tested and was not significant. Hypothesis 3. Hypothesis 3 predicted a significant main effect for experience in predicting role expansion. It was predicted that when subjects experienced several jobs within the team, this would lead to greater role expansion. Although experience had a 73 74 Table 6: Means and standard deviations of variables in the study. Standard Variable Mean Deviation E Consideration 2.49 1 . 10 240 Initiating Structure" 2.0] 1.00 240 Pre Task Knowledge Test 9.80 3.28 240 Role Expansion 22.92 15.00 240 Decision Making Performance 1 .36 .54 240 * Square root transformation was used to normalized the skewed distribution and the transformed values were used in all analyses involving initiating structure. 75 Table 7: Intercorrelations among the variables in the study. 1 2 3 4 5 6 l-C 2-IS .16** 3-PTKT -.01 -.01 4-RE .25“ -.02 .00 5-DMP .02 .09* .02 -.O8 6-EXP -.l7** .02 .33“ -.31** .20** 7-FDBK -.04 .14” -.06 -.08 .01 .00 8-STR .00 .00 .00 .00 .17** .00 .00 C = Consideration IS = Initiating Structure PTKT = Pre-Task Knowledge Test RE = Role Expansion DMP = Decision Making Performance EXP = Experience (1=high, 0=low) FDBK = Feedback (1=process, O=outcome) STR = Stress (1=high, 0=low) * < .05 ** < .01 76 Table 8: Regression results with role expansion as the dependant variable. flap Variable B R R} R2 Change 1. Consideration 3.38 .247 .061 .061“ 2. Initiating Structure -2.67 .255 .065 .004 3. ConsidXIS 1.32 .257 .066 .001 4. Experience -9.18 .395 .156 .090” 5. Feedback -1.63 .398 .159 .003 6. EprFeedback 4.04 .404 .163 .004 7. EprConsid .02 .404 .163 .000 8. FeedbackXConsid -.72 .404 .163 .000 9. EprIS -.40 .404 .163 .000 10. FeedbackXIS -16.24 .441 .195 .032" *p<.05 **p<.01 77 Figure 19: Main effect of the leader's behavior of consideration on role expansion. 29: """"" q “““““““““““““““““““““ 27 25 Role Expansion to 0) 21 17 LOW Leader Consideration High 78 main effect on the amount of role expansion accounting for 9% of the variance (R2 = .09, p < .01), it was not in the predicted direction (see Figure 20). Those subjects who experienced several jobs expanded their roles significantly less than did subjects who experienced only one job throughout the experiment. Hypothesis 4. Hypothesis 4 predicted a significant main effect for the type of feedback that role incumbents received on role expansion. Specifically, it was predicted that role incumbents who received process feedback would expand their roles more than subjects who received only outcome feedback. The type of feedback was not significant, accounting for only .3% of the variance in role expansion (R2 = .003, ns p < .05). The interaction between experience and the type of feedback was tested and was not significant. Hypothesis 5. Hypothesis 5 predicted a significant interaction between the amount of experience a role incumbent had and the leader's level of consideration in the prediction of role expansion. This interaction was not significant. Hypothesis 6. Hypothesis 6 predicted a significant interaction between the type of feedback a role incumbent received and the leader's level on consideration in the prediction of role expansion. This interaction was not significant. Hypothesis 7. Hypothesis 7 predicted a significant interaction between the amount of experience role incumbents received and the leader's level of initiating structure in the prediction of role expansion. This interaction was not significant. Hypothesis 8. Hypothesis 8 predicted a Significant interaction between the type of feedback role incumbents received and the leader's level of initiating structure in the prediction of role expansion. This interaction was significant accounting for 2.6% of the 79 Figure 20: Main effect of job experience on role expansion. 29 25: .f Role Expansion h) 0 lo .a LOW Job Experience High 80 variance (R2 = .026, p < .01). However, Figure 21 shows that feedback did not make the leader's level of initiating structure irrelevant. Process feedback resulted in less role expansion when the leader's level of initiating structure was high. It was predicted that high initiating structure would not have an effect in the process feedback condition (see Figure 8). Analysis of the Decision Making Accuracwypotheses The next two hypotheses refer to the second part of the study which deals with decision making accuracy. In the first part the focus was on role expansion; in the second part the focus is on how role expansion effects decision making accuracy. The moderator regression analysis used to test the last two hypotheses included trials in the last half of the experiment (trials 21 - 36). The first four variables entered into the regression equation were role expansion, job knowledge, stress and the interaction between job knowledge and stress. There were no predictions made about the effect of these variables on decision making performance. Table 9 shows that only stress had a main effect on decision making performance. Stress accounted for 2.9% of the variance (R2 = .029, p < .01). Figure 22 Shows that when stress was high decision making performance was significantly worse than when Stress was low. Role expansion, feedback and the interaction between incumbent job knowledge and stress were not significant. Hypothesis 9. Hypothesis 9 predicted a significant interaction between role expansion and job knowledge in predicting decision making accuracy. The nature of the interaction is such that when role expansion is high and task knowledge is low, decision making accuracy will be especially low. Also, when role expansion is high and task 81 Table 9: Regression results with decision making accuracy as the dependant variable. Step Variable R R R} R2 Change 1. Role Expansion (RE) -.003 .076 .006 .006+ 2. Job Knowledge (JK) .004 .079 .006 .001 3. Stress (Str) .186 .190 .036 .030" 4. JK_Str .013 .194 .038 .002 5. RE_JK -.005 .196 .038 .001 6. RE_STR -.027 .197 .039 .000 +p