f a"! f 5 I." I P.7'.J‘ I'm“; ‘; t‘fffifa’f—v -’ ff? {53 1X . vs ‘0’ ' x‘ r .52 V 'R‘ . ”1 V- ' i‘ "34.’;;,-_u.. f-‘lfimv J, f: L . x v": K5?) ‘5 . 2’.“ ,9 H u '2 ‘va - . , ‘5” 3%... "N’ ‘n- ~ ”i cgfi'fi-vam «F "ma-35a”: ‘ .V‘. '~”‘”‘*‘ ‘ “In; A.“ "f V ,4 ‘ ‘ E2331: ‘. WU ‘ ‘ _ ‘ a? k “3% CL: — 3; : - exé”: w . V dd):- ‘ 6‘13? ~ l ”*E‘Tx’f-‘FLxfi ‘. . 1‘- ‘7‘? ‘1 ~ g m; V“ \ th‘;‘...~..¢ ' ‘- W‘g- . ’zfiizfi‘s: ; in ‘ ékfiiilg‘jn r fared-r . ‘n~ $4- ». a .‘tflr'hfif‘ j ‘ ‘mr ~ «.1 ‘ R} , 1:. , . N; . f. rt. - 1 '.a 1-7. ‘ ‘Wf'o‘ : '.. “3?" ' a . $1,: 31%v1 :1) r . . ”13'?“ . z. ‘ ‘ "A" "J "‘7‘ ~ . Ev"; . ' 333% . . ‘ ,4 N ” “2w . ‘ &}€?%§m%§ggkhz :9," . 1 "; ‘: fig“ 44% W 3, fi%* :3. :2? {a 3; " 7%. '33:“ " wiéw‘fiz" ‘; f 1.1" 2 I t‘ k A " .- 4, h :V'rb, N , ‘2‘“! 3-42; 1?: “$5555" gii'év . 1 UNIVERSITY LIBRARIE lllllllllllillllllllMilli ll'lllllll 3 1293 01029 884 Helllill This is to certify that the dissertation entitled ECOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF DIET-INDUCED THERMOGENESIS IN THE PRAIRIE VOLE, MIQBOTUS OQERQQASTER presented by Terry Martin Trier has been accepted towards fulfillment ' of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein Zoology 6 ajor ' fessor Date 1/12/94 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LIBRARY MIohIgan State Untverslty PLACE IN RETURN BOX to roman this ohockout Iron your rooord. To AVOID FINES rotum on or baton m duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE itg‘f) ’fif‘fi I w. MSU In An Afflrmottvo AotIoNEmol Opportunity Imtttmlon ' W ”3-9.! ECOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF DIET-INDUCED TI-IERMOGENESIS IN THE PRAIRIE VOLE, MICROTUS OCHROGASIER By Terry Martin Trier A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Zoology 1994 ABSTRACT ECOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF DIET-INDUCED THERMOGENESIS IN THE PRAIRIE VOLE, MICROTUS OCHROGASTER By Terry Martin Trier Diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT) occurs in laboratory rats consuming either a cafeteria or a low-protein diet. Researchers suggest that DIT regulates weight by oxidizing excess food intake. However, DIT may enhance nutrient uptake from a diet that is nutritionally unbalanced. By selective oxidation of excess nutrients, the diet can become more balanced and uptake of one or more limiting nutrients could increase. Also, a hyperphagic response to an unbalanced diet can increase nutrient intake, and excess nonessential nutrients can be oxidized via DIT. In the wild, nitrogen can be in limited supply for many animals, especially herbivores. Ecological studies of herbivores indicate that many herbivores may periodically consume low-nitrogen diets which can limit growth and reproduction. Therefore, natural selection should favor the evolution of DIT in herbivores to enhance nitrogen uptake from diets that are typically low in nitrogen. To test the hypothesis that DIT occurs in small, mammalian herbivores and may augment nitrogen uptake, wild herbivorous prairie voles were studied to see if 1) DIT occurs in voles consuming a low-protein diet, 2) the response is mediated by brown adipose tissue (BAT), the putative effector organ of DIT, 3) hyperphagia occurs in response to low-protein feeding, and 4) body protein is conserved and lipid deposition is low, suggesting regulation of body protein and lipid levels during low—protein fwding. Energy balance studies of voles fed semipurified diets showed that low-protein- fed voles (2.5 % casein diet) had lower energy efficiencies and higher metabolic rates per unit of metabolizable energy intake than high-protein-fed voles (15 % casien diet), which is indicative of DIT. Daily food consumption increased for voles on low- protein diets and decreased for voles on high-protein diets over a three-week period. Both high- and low-protein groups deposited dry tissue (lipid, protein, and other) in the same relative proportions, although total dry tissue depositon was greater for the high-protein group for each constituent. GDP binding activity did not increase in the BAT of low-protein-fed voles, suggesting that BAT may not be involved in low- protein DIT observed in voles. To my mother, Betty Rebecca Rhoades iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My Sincere thanks to the members of my committee for their support and understanding. I especially want to thank Dale Romsos for his invaluable advice and technical support and for allowing me to use his laboratory, equipment, and supplies. Also, special thanks go to Nancy Soloman, who provided the prairie voles used to start my colony, Hsiao-Ling Chen, who helped with the GDP binding assay, and Virginia Vega-Vargas, who helped with the Kjeldahl analysis. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ..................................... LIST OF FIGURES ..................................... INTRODUCTION ...................................... CHAPTER I. DIET-INDUCED THERMOGENESIS ................ THE CAFETERIA DIET AND DIT ......................... THE CAFETERIA DIET AND BAT-MEDIATED DIT ............... Low-PROTEIN DIETS AND DIT .......................... Nutrient Composition and Thermogenesis ............... Recent DIT Research Using Low-Protein Diets ............ CONTROVERSY AND DIT .............................. Heat Increment of Feeding and DIT .................. The Case Against DIT .......................... ECOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK FOR DIT RESEARCH .................... CHAPTER II. ECOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK .................... THE HERBIVORE TROPHIC LEVEL ........................ RELATIVE FOOD SHORTAGE ............................ Change in Plant Quality and Its Influence on Herbivores ...... TEMPORAL HETEROGENEITY ................... SECONDARY PLANT CHEMICALS ................ STRUCTURAL DEFENSE ...................... INDUCED DEFENSE ........................ SUMMARY ...................................... Herbivore Response to 3 Variable Food Supply ........... INFLUENCE OF SUPPLEMENTAL FOOD ............. FORAGE QUALITY ......................... COMPENSATORY MECHANISMS ................. Summary ................ * .................. METABOLIC RATE AND THE NUTRITIONAL ECOLOGY OF HERBIVORES . . Food Habits and Energetics in Mammals ............... vi Page ix 10 12 14 15 16 16 18 19 23 24 29 30 3O 35 49 50 52 53 53 56 57 62 63 CHAPTER II (continued). Page Food Habits and Energetic Efficiencies in Invertebrates ....... 71 Thermogenic Mechanisms and Dietary Protein ............ 73 CONCLUSION ..................................... 74 CHAPTER III. EXPERIMENTS AND ANALYSIS ................. 75 INTRODUCTION ................................... 75 EXPERIMENT 1 .................................... 79 Introduction ................................. 79 Materials and Methods .......................... 79 DETERMINATION OF BODY ENERGY .............. 82 Results .................................... 83 EXPERIMENT 2 .................................... 85 Introduction ................................. 85 Materials and Methods .......................... 86 Results .................................... 87 EXPERIMENT 3 .................................... 89 Introduction ................................. 89 Materials and Methods .......................... 90 Results .................................... 91 EXPERIMENT 4 .................................... 94 Introduction ................................. 94 Materials and Methods .......................... 94 Results .................................... 96 EXPERIMENT 5 .................................... 96 Introduction ................................. 96 Materials and Methods .......................... 98 Results .................................... 98 EXPERIMENT 6 .................................... 99 Introduction ................................. 99 Materials and Methods .......................... 100 Results .................................... 100 EXPERIMENT 7 .................................... 101 Introduction ................................. 101 Materials and Methods .......................... 102 Results .................................... 107 FOOD CONSUMPTION ....................... 107 BODY COMPOSITION ....................... 109 vii CHAPTER HI (continued). Page NITROGEN BALANCE ....................... 113 DISCUSSION ..................................... 113 Introduction ................................. 113 Thermogenesis ............................... 1 14 Brown Adipose Tissue ........................... 120 Food Consumption ............................. 125 Body Composition ............................. 129 Nitrogen Balance .............................. 130 CHAPTER IV. CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS ......... 132 LIST OF REFERENCES .................................. 144 viii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1.1 Influence of low-protein diets on parameters associated with an in- crease in thermogenesis .............................. 15 2.1 Influence of supplemental food on reproduction in hares and micro- tines .......................................... 54 2.2 Influence of supplemental food on growth in hares and microtines . . . . 56 3.1 Composition of diets, expt. 1 ........................... 80 3.2 Metabolic rate, energy intake, and retention, expt. 1 ............. 83 3.3 Composition of diets, expt. 2 ........................... 86 3.4 Metabolic rate, energy intake, and retention, expt. 2 ............. 88 3.5 Composition of diets, expt. 3 ........................... 90 3.6 Metabolic rate, energy intake, and retention, expt. 3 ............. 92 3.7 Total and weight-specific food consumption, expt. 7 ............. 105 3. 8 Post hoc polynomial contrasts, expt. 7 ..................... 107 3.9 Body composition of voles fed 2.5%, 5%, and 15% casein diets, expt. 7 ........................................ 108 3.10 Statistical analysis of selected body composition data listed in table 3.9, expt. 7 ..................................... 109 3.11 % Composition of final wet and dry weights, expt. 7 ............ 110 3.12 Gross nitrogen balance, expt. 7 ......................... 112 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1 Lipid metabolism and the proton conductance pathway in BAT ...... 1.2 Heat increments of variable protein diets .................... 2.1 Thomas’ model ................................... 2.2 Relationship between food nitrogen concentration and the efficiency of conversion of ingested food (ECI) by assorted invertebrate herbivores 3.1 Distribution of Microtus ochrogaster ...................... 3.2 The fate of ingested energy in an organism .................. 3.3 Regression of the ADMR of low- and high- protein groups on the co- variate ME to test for homogeneity of Slopes in an analysis of covari- ance (ANCOVA) design .............................. 3.4 Mean daily growth of weanling voles, expt. 2 ................. 3.5 Mean daily growth of weanling voles, expt. 3 ................. 3.6 General design of feeders used in expt. 7 .................... 3.7 Mean daily food consumption, expt. 7 ..................... 3.8 Weight-specific mean daily food consumption, expt. 7 ............ 3.9 Growth of weanling voles, expt. 7 ........................ 3.10 Efficiency of energy retention, expt. 3 ..................... 3.11 GDP binding, expts. 4 and 6 ........................... 3.12 Mean total food consumption for 21 days of expt. 7 ............ 14 72 75 84 89 93 103 106 106 111 117 122 Figure Page 3.13 Mean total food consumption for the first 10 days of expt. 7‘ ........ 126 3.14 Mean weight-specific total food consumption for 21 days of expt. 7 . . . 126 3.15 Mean weight-specific total food consumption for the first 10 days of expt. 7 ....................................... 126 4.1 Efficiency of enrgy retention, expt. 3 ...................... 141 4.2 GDP binding, expts. 4 and 6 ........................... 141 4.3 Gross nitrogen efficiencies, expt. 7 ....................... 141 4.4 Growth of weanling voles, expt. 7 ........................ 141 4.5 Mean daily food consumption, expt. 7 ..................... 142 4.6 Total mean food intake per vole for 10 and 21 days during expt. 7 . . . . 142 4.7 Data are for weight changes in tissue constituents over a 21-day feed- ing period during expt. 7 ............................. 142 4.8 Relative proportions of dry tissue constituents for voles at the end of expt. 7 ........................................ 143 xi INTRODUCTION The main purpose of my research is to examine ecological implications of a physiological phenomenon known as diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT). Since 1979 when Rothwell and Stock published a paper in Nature where they proposed that diet and brown fat metabolism are linked, DIT has been the focus of intense research by nutritionists and biochemists whose main interest has been to determine the effects of DIT on obesity in humans. However, the ecological implications of this phenomenon for wild animals have yet to be investigated. DIT is a phenomenon that may have considerable relevance to the nutritional ecology and bioenergetics of wild animals, and therefore Should be examined within an ecological framework. AS the amount of research on DIT continues to grow, researchers are begin- ning to make claims that have not been explored within the context of ecology and evolutionary biology. Rothwell and Stock (1986a) believe that DIT is an adaptive trait and Should be labeled as such. Rothwell and Stock ( 1986a) wrote “. . . we have opted for the terms obligatory and adaptive diet-induced thermogenesis to differentiate between the two influences of the net availability of metabolizable energy.” Similar- ly, according to Trayhum and Milner (1987), “Diet-induced thermogenesis (currently the most widely used expression) and luxusconsumption are terms which are primarily used to describe an adaptive energy-dissipative process.” 2 According to the claims of Rothwell and Stock (1986a) and Trayhum and Milner (1987), DIT is an adaptive process. However, one of the problems associated with labeling a term as adaptive is that the word ‘adaptive’ has several meanings in the sciences, and therefore can lead to ambiguity. The following statement by Gould and Lewontin (1979) summarizes this problem: ‘Adaptation’ “the good fit of organisms to their environment” can occur at three hierarchical levels with different causes. It is unfortunate that our language has focused on the common result and called all three phenome- na ‘adaptation’: the differences in process have been obscured and evolution- ists have often been misled to extend the Darwinian mode to the other two levels as well. First, we have what physiologists call ‘adaptation’: the pheno- typic plasticity that permits organisms to mould their form to prevailing circumstances during ontogeny. Human ‘adaptations’ to high altitude fall into this category (while others, like resistance of sickling heterozygotes to malar- ia, are genetic and Darwinian). Physiological adaptations are not heritable, though the capacity to develop them presumably is. Secondly, we have a ‘heritable’ form of non-Darwinian adaptation in humans (and, in rudimentary ways, in a few other advanced social species): cultural adaptation (with heritability imposed by learning). Much confused drinking in human sociobiology arises from a failure to distinguish this mode from Darwinian adaptation based on genetic variation. Finally, we have adaptation arising from the conventional Darwinian mechanism of selection upon genetic varia- tion. The mere existence of a good fit between organism and environment is insufficient evidence for inferring the action of natural selection. 3 Since the term adaptation can be interpreted in at least three different ways, the question arises as to the precise meaning of the term ‘adaptive DIT.’ While most human nutritionists are primarily interested in the study of physiological adaptations (Waterlow 1985), it is clear that Rothwell and Stock (1981a) are labeling adaptive DIT as a Darwinian adaptation. Rothwell and Stock (1981a) argue that DIT is adaptive because it preserves leanness in wild animals. According to Rothwell and Stock (1981a), obesity in wild animals would impair their locomotor activity and reduce their ability to hunt for food or avoid capture. Also, Rothwell and Stock ( 1981a) argue that obesity could reduce reproductive capacity in wild animals. Sterility, complications associated with pregnancy, and high perinatal mortality are traits that are commonly associated with obesity in laboratory rodents. Therefore, according to Rothwell and Stock ( 1981a), those animals that can preserve leanness by DIT and thus avoid obesity, would have higher fecundity and increased survivorship in the wild. Although Rothwell and Stock believe DI’T is a Darwinian adaptation, it is not clear that other researchers who study DIT use the term in similar fashion, thus creating confusion. Moreover, by labeling DIT adaptive, researchers are introducing a nonneutral term (‘adaptive DIT’) into the literature. Putting nonneutral labels on terms can often lead to bias and stifle alternative research. If we call DIT adaptive without further research to understand its role in nature (or lack of one), then the term ‘adaptive DIT’ may become fixed in the literature and could hinder our ability to think objectively about the occurrence and function of DIT. Therefore, although DIT 4 may be an adaptive trait, until we know more about it, putting an adaptive label on DIT should probably be avoided. If we label DIT as adaptive, then we should be able to state clearly and with little ambiguity, how it is adaptive. However, several adaptive claims have been made for DIT, and it is not clear which, if any, are justified. HimmS-Hagen (1986) summarized some of the more significant claims as follows: “The function of diet-induced thermogenesis in the rat is suggested to be 3-fold. First, it contributes, together with cold-induced nonshivering thermogenesis, to the maintenance of eu— thermia in the cold. Second, because much of the excess food is used for increased thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue, the development of obesity due to hyperphagia is attenuated and mobility of the animal is preserved. Third, intake of nutrients can be increased when the diet is low in protein so as to achieve an adequate protein intake without the consequent development of obesity due to the increased energy intake.” These proposals are interesting and deserve serious consideration. Howev- er, as far as I know, none of the proposals above has ever been fully examined within an ecological or evolutionary framework, where specific selective pressures known to occur in nature which might favor the evolution of DIT are rigorously examined. Therefore, from an ecological perspective, there is little support for labeling DIT as adaptive. If DIT is to be considered an adaptive trait, its adaptive function should be readily understood within an ecological context. If we assume DIT is an adaptation that evolved by natural selection, then we should be able to identify the selective pres- sures that led to the evolution of DIT. However, the key factors in nature that may 5 have favored the evolution of DIT have not been rigorously defined. Rothwell and Stock (1981a) have discussed the evolution of DIT as an adaptive trait that prevents obesity in animals, but their treatment of this topic was not extensive and, in their own words, their arguments are based on “apocryphal sources and ex cathedra statements” and characterized by “gross extrapolations and partisan view. ” Their intention, however, was not to provide a rigorous ecological framework for the evolution of DIT, but to stimulate discussion (Rothwell and Stock 1981a). It is not the goal of my research to prove or disprove DIT as an adaptive trait. That may require many years of research. But it is my goal to take tentative Steps in that direction. According to Mayr (1983), “. . . when one attempts to explain the features of something that is the product of evolution, one must attempt to reconstruct the evolutionary history of this feature. This can be done only by inference. ” There- fore, one of the goals of my research is to identify ecological factors that might have favored the evolution of DIT. An ecological framework discussing the factors that may have led to a selective advantage for the evolution of DIT is covered extensively in Chapter II. The ecological framework presented in Chapter 11, however, represents a change from the focus on DIT as a weight regulatory mechanism, to one where DIT is postulated to be an adaptation to enhance nutrient uptake in animals that may be limited by food quality in the wild. According to this hypothesis, the main function of DI’I‘ is not weight regulation, although perhaps that might be a proximate effect. Instead, the hypothesized functional significance of DIT is to permit wild animals that normally feed on food low in quality to ingest large amounts of food and burn off 6 excess calories while distilling essential nutrients. More specifically, for many herbivores, nitrogen may be the main limiting nutrient in their forage in the wild (Chapter H), and DIT could function to increase nitrogen intake. According to Mayr (1983), one of the first Steps in demonstrating that a trait is adaptive “ . . . consists in establishing a tentative correlation between a trait and a feature of the environment . . .” Thus, for this research, a correlation must be established between low-nitrogen food intake by herbivores and a DIT response, before DIT can be seriously considered as an adaptive trait that enhances nutrient uptake in wild herbivores. This research begins by predicting that, if DIT is adaptive in aiding nitrogen uptake, then it should occur in wild herbivores that may be limited by low-nitrogen forage. To investigate the occurrence of DIT in wild mammals, prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) were used in my research. Prairie voles were chosen because they are primarily herbivorous and consume large amounts of low-nitrogen foodstuffs to satisfy their nutritional needs in the wild (Batzli 1985). If DIT is an adaptive trait that enhances nitrogen intake in small herbivores such as the prairie vole, we would expect to see a DIT response when prairie voles consume diets low in protein. Thus, much of my research involves measuring energy efficiencies and metabolic rates of voles consuming diets that contain different levels of protein. Also, food consumption rates were investigated to see if voles exhibit hyper- phagia when fed low-protein diets. A hyperphagic response and a concomitant DIT response to a low-protein fwding diet would suggest that voles oxidize excess caloric intake and increase nitrogen uptake by distillation of nitrogen from the increased food 7 intake. Gross nitrogen efficiency, body tissue change (lipid, protein, water and ‘other’), and biochemical changes in brown fat, a heat-producing tissue thought to be the primary effector organ of NST, were also studied in voles consuming diets with variable protein content. Their relevance to this research is discussed in Chapter III. In general, my research was undertaken to see if DIT could be observed in wild mammals and understood within the framework of their nutritional ecology. The rest of the dissertation is divided by chapters containing the following information: Chapter I is a brief review of DIT research and Chapter H presents an evolutionary and ecologically-based raison d ’étre for DIT. A detailed discussion of my research goals and specific hypotheses tested are presented in Chapter III; research protocol, experimental results, and a discussion of the results are also covered in Chapter III. Concluding remarks and suggestions for further research are given in Chapter IV. CHAPTER I DIET-INDUCED THERMOGENESIS Measurement of energy balance can be used to determine the efficiency of use of metabolizable energy (ME) available in the food that an animal consumes. Energy balance is the difference between the intake and expenditure of energy by an organism over time (Hervey and Tobin 1982). In order for an animal to achieve zero energy balance, energy intake and expenditure must be equal. If an animal increases the amount of heat it produces for a given amount of ME, then efficiency and energy balance are lowered. Conversely, if an animal reduces heat production and stores more energy in the body for a given amount of ME, then efficiency is increased and energy balance becomes more positive. Diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT), as defined by Rothwell and Stock (1986a), refers to a physiological phenomenon that regulates energy balance for an animal by increasing its metabolic rate in response to an increase in food intake above the level required for maintenance. The result is that an animal can preserve its body weight over a range of food intake levels by burning ‘Surplus’ calories when food consumption is in excess. The idea of homeostatic regulation of energy balance and body weight extends back to the turn of the century when the German physiologists Voit and Neumann suggested that the body could eliminate excess energy ingestion directly as heat (a phenomenon called luxuskonsumption) (Voit 1901, and Neumann 1902, as cited by 9 Rothwell and Stock 1987a). Neumann experimented on himself and showed that he could maintain his body weight at a constant level for an extended period of time, even though his daily caloric intake varied. Gulick (1922) performed a Similar experiment and achieved comparable results. THE CAFETERIA DIET AND DIT Current interest in the DIT phenomenon stems from research by Miller and Payne (1962), who showed that pigs on a low-protein diet fed ad libitum consumed five times more energy than pigs on an energy-restricted high-protein diet, with no difference in weight gain. The experiment was repeated in 1980 by Gurr et al., who reported that, compared to the high-protein group, there was a two-fold increase in energy consumption and expenditure for pigs fed low-protein diets. Rothwell and Stock (1979) began using a highly palatable ‘cafeteria’ or ‘junk food’ diet in order to stimulate excess food consumption (hyperphagia) in rats. This allowed them to compare two groups of animals fed ad libitum. Different levels of food intake occurred as a result of the hyperphagic response exhibited by the group fed the cafeteria diet, thus creating an increase in caloric intake in the cafeteria—fed (CAF) group. Their results, published in Nature in 1979, provided the impetus for a wide variety of research by nutritionists and biochemists into the existence and causal mechanism of DIT. Rothwell and Stock (1979) found that rats consuming a cafeteria diet ingested almost twice the energy as controls fed a stock diet, but efficiency of energy retention was much lower. Furthermore, CAF rats had an increased sensitivity to norepineph- 10 Tine-induced thermogenesis, which was determined by measuring oxygen consumption (170,) after an injection of norepinephrine (NE); V0, declined when CAF rats were treated with the B-adrenergic blocking agent propranolol. The control group exhibited only a slight response to treatments. These results showed a striking similarity to those observed in animals exhibiting cold-induced non-shivering thermogenesis (NST) (see Jansky 1973), a phenomenon thought to be mediated primarily by the sympathetic nervous system and effected by brown adipose tissue (BAT) (Foster and Frydman 1978). This led Rothwell and Stock (1979) to propose that DIT elicited by cafeteria feeding is a result of sympathetic activation of thermogenesis in BAT. THE CAFETERIA DIET AND BAT-MEDIATED DIT Numerous studies have been conducted to test Rothwell and Stock’s claim. Evidence supporting their proposal is extensive and varied. BAT hypertrophy, hyperplasia, and an increase in protein content have been observed in CAF rats (Himms-Hagen, Triandafillou, and Gwilliam 1981; Rothwell, Saville, and Stock 1982a; Goldberg and Morgan 1983; Rothwell, Stock, and Warwick 1983b). The specific mitochondrial protein thermogenin increases in concentration in CAF rats. Thermogenin is a 32 kD protein thought to be responsible for the uncoupling of ATP production from electron transport, resulting in the dissipation of ME as heat via a proton—conductance pathway across the inner mitochondrial membrane (Figure 1.1). Use of a cDNA probe provides evidence that there is also a concomitant increase in thermogenin mRNA (Falcou et a1. 1985). In vitro binding of GDP to BAT mitochondrial protein, considered a good parameter for measuring the activity of the 11 proton-conductance pathway (Nicholls 1979), is elevated in CAF rats (Nedergaard, Raasmaja, and Cannon 1984; Brooks, Rothwell, and Stock 1980). Noradrmaline I plmamhnne CAMP ——» hn' aces “‘0 «mum» + FFA H amm- (-) ACam ’+ M+ I i moron cououcrmce ACoA —> p-orddatlon PATHWAY Figure 1.1. Lipid metabolism and the proton conductance pathway in BAT. ACoA: acetyl coenzyme A; ACam: Acetyl-camitine; ATP, AMP, and GDP: purine nucleotides; CAMP: cyclic adenosine-S’-monophosphate; FFA: free fatty acids; TCA: tricarboxylic acid cycle; TG: triglyceride droplet; 32K: thermogenin uncoupling protein. (Adapted from Bukowiecki 1986, and Nicholls and Rial 1988.) An increase in the rate of norepinephrine turnover in BAT resulting from cafeteria fwding reinforces the idea of sympathetic regulation of DIT (Young et a1. 1982). Mitochondrial enzyme activities (cytochrome oxidase and a-glycerophosphate 12 dehydrogenase) in BAT of CAP rats are more than twice the levels measured in controls (Brooks et al. 1980), indicating an increase in cellular respiration in this tissue. Additionally, many studies of CAF rats report a hyperphagic response and simultaneous decline in energetic efficiency (see Rothwell and Stock 1986a, for a review). Thus, the findings of Rothwell and Stock (1979) are supported quantitatively by evidence gathered at the molecular, cellular, and organismal level. Further evidence supporting the claim that BAT-mediated DIT is involved in energy balance comes from experiments using genetically obese laboratory rodents. GDP binding to BAT mitochondrial protein is reduced in obese (ob/ob) mice, and binding does not increase upon cold exposure (Himms-Hagen and Desautels 1978). GDP binding is also reduced in adult, genetically obese Zucker rats (fa/fa), and both fa/fa rats and ob/ob mice show lower thermogenin concentrations than their lean siblings (Ashwell et al. 1985). Also, lesions in the ventromedial region of the hypothalamus cause obesity in rats (Hogan, Coscina, and Himms-Hagen 1982; Vander Tuig, Kemer, and Romsos 1985). This is significant because the hypothalamus is implicated in the control of thermogenesis in BAT via sympathetic innervation (Landsberg and Young 1983). Low-PROTEIN DIETS AND DIT The term ‘diet-induced thermogenesis’ also has been applied to an observed decrease in the efficiency of energy retention in response to consumption of a low- protein diet. In some instances, an increase in food consumption and concomitant decrease in efficiency for animals on a low-protein diet have been recorded (Miller 13 and Payne 1962; Gurr et al. 1980). In those experiments, animals on low-protein diets were fed ad libitum, with a mixture of ingredients (protein, fat, and carbohydrates) sufficient to maintain body weight; those in the high-protein group were on an energy-restricted, weight-maintenance diet. In other experiments, where animals were fed ad libitum for both low- and high-protein diets, food consumption for low-protein-fed animals was the same or, more often, lower than those fed high—protein diets, although weight-specific food consumption was usually higher (Tulp et al. 1979; Donald, Pitts, and Pohl 1981; Rothwell, Stock, and Tyzbir 1982b, 1983a; Kevonian, Vander Tuig, and Romsos 1984; Rothwell and Stock 1987b). One notable exception, though, was a study by Stirling and Stock (1968) who found that rats fed low-protein diets for 20 days consumed 69 percent more energy than their counterparts fed high-protein rations. The results of the studies mentioned above indicate that when both low- and high-protein diets are fed ad libitum, DIT can be elicited in animals that are not hyperphagic, but rather hypophagic (based on gross food consumption). Since CAF rats are given a choice of foods, the cafeteria diet has been criticized for being uncontrolled in terms of nutrient composition (Moore 1987; see Rothwell and Stock 1988, for reply). Moore (1987) postulated that the DIT phenomenon is due primarily to the consumption of a diet that is unbalanced in nutrient composition and has proposed that for future energy balance studies of DIT, palatable, nutrient-controlled diets should be used instead of cafeteria diets. 14 15 I I l I 1 E I Ero- i N 5 55' II S u: o I I I I I 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 i PROTEIN IN DIET Figure 1.2. Heat increments of variable protein diets. (Data from Hamilton 1939a.) Nutrient Composition and Thermogenesis That unbalanced diets cause an increase in thermogenesis was noted by Rubner (1902, as cited by Glick 1987), who called the phenomenon “specific dynamic effect” because the specific response to a protein meal was higher than for a carbohydrate or fat meal. In 1934, Mitchell stated that it was not protein per se that caused the increase in metabolic rate; food consumption, in general, has a thermic effect. Instead, Mitchell declared, it is a ‘balanced diet’ that produces the smallest amount of heat. This claim was substantiated by Hamilton (1939a) using laboratory rats. Hamilton demonstrated that diets with low- and high-protein content had the greatest thermic effect, while those with intermediate protein levels had the least (Figure 1.2). 15 Kleiber (1945) reviewed this topic and concluded that a dietary deficiency of any nutrient (vitamins, minerals, protein, etc.) would alter energy metabolism and stated that “ . . . a diet is deficient in any nutrient whose addition decreases the calorigenic effect of the ration. ” Discussing low-protein diets, Kleiber Stated that “One of the homeostatic reactions of an animal against the effect of a low protein diet may be an increase in the combustion of carbohydrates and fats. ” Recent DIT Research Using Low-Protein Diets A variety of studies (summarized in Table 1.1) indicate that a thermogenic response resulting from the consumption of a low-protein diet is similar in kind to that invoked by cafeteria feeding. Some studies implicate BAT as an effector organ and the sympathetic nervous system as a mediator of low-protein DIT. Thus, DIT can be induced by cafeteria feeding and by consumption of a low-protein diet, but it is not Table 1.1. Influence of low-protein diets on parameters associated with an increase in thermogenesis. Response to low-protein diet Reference Increase in thermic response' and decrease in Tulp et al. 1979; Rothwell et al. 1983a; Kevonian efficiency of energy retention et al. 1984; Swick and Swick 1986; Rothwell and Stock 1987c Increase in GDP binding to BAT mitochondrial Rothwell et al. 1983a; Swick and Gribskov 1983; protein Swick and Swick 1986; Rothwell and Stock 19870; Swick and Henningfield 1989 Increase in V0, response to NE Stirling and Stock 1968; Kevonian et al. 1984; Rothwell and Stock 1987c Increase in NE turnover in BAT Kevonian et al. 1984; Vander Tuig and Romsos 1984; Young et al. 1985 Decline in V0, in response to B—adrenergic blockade Rothwell et al. 1983a Increase in plasma thyroid hormones2 Tulp et al. 1979 1Refers to heat production per unit metabolizable energy. 2This hormonal change my affect the thermogenic responsiveness of BAT (Kopecky et al. 1986). 16 clear whether these are the same or different phenomena. Research into DIT has been motivated primarily by its applicability to energy balance in humans and relevance to weight control and obesity. However, since it can be induced under hypophagic conditions, the physiological significance of DIT may be multifaceted, and not related to energy balance alone. My research centers on DIT induced by low—protein diets and its ecological implications for animals living in the wild. CONTROVERSY AND DIT The idea that there is an increase in heat production that accompanies an increase in food intake is well established in the nutrition literature. The monogas- tric, or Rubner school of nutritionists called this phenomenon specific dynamic effect, while the Kellner, or ruminant school called it heat increment of feeding (HIF) (Webster 1981). Both terms refer approximately to the same event, differing mainly in the length of time the measurements of heat production are taken. The measure- ments are usually taken right after the consumption of a meal and, therefore, the thermogenic response has also been called the “thermic effect of a meal” by Gar- row (1978). Heat Increment of Feeding and DIT The mechanism of HIF (HIF is used because it is a more neutral term than specific dynamic effect) continues to be a fertile topic for debate in the nutrition literature, and the discussion is now further complicated by the phenomenon of DIT, which may or may not be related to HIF (Glick, Teague, and Bray 1981; Glick 1987). Historically, HIF has been considered an acute response to normal food l7 consumption; DIT is regarded as a chronic response that dissipates excess food consumption resulting from long-term overeating (Glick 1987). Mechanisms for HIF have been ascribed to a variety of processes (see Kleiber 1975a, for a historical review). Webster (1983) delineates the essential (but not necessarily only) contributors to HIF in the following statement: “. . . they must include the energy costs of ingestion and digestion of food, the work of active absorption and secretion of substances across the gut wall, increments in H1 in the food processing tissues such as the gut wall and the liver associated with increasing inflow of substrates and the costs of synthesis (turnover and net accretion) in body tissues, muscle, fat, mammary gland, etc. All of these things must be considered as inevitable contributors to HIF. ” The difference between HIF and DIT is that the amount of heat that is produced by DIT is considered to be much greater than what can be accounted for by the processes described by Webster. Because the multitude of terms designating dietary heat production in animals has led to some confusion, Rothwell and Stock ( 1986a) proposed that the terms obligatory and adaptive DIT be used to distinguish between the unavoidable (obligato- ry) costs of metabolizing foodstuffs, e. g., those described by Webster (1983) above, and nonessential (adaptive) thermogenesis, which presumably dissipates excess energy. Adaptive DIT represents heat production that cannot be accounted for by metabolic work associated with processing of food. This terminology is convenient because, in some ways, adaptive DIT is a more neutral term than specific dynamic effect (referring to the specific effect of protein) or luxuskonsumption (referring to 1H refers to metabolic heat production (Webster 1983). 18 hyperphagic consumption). However, as I have suggested in the Introduction, there are good reasons for avoiding this type of characterization, because what appears to be an unbiased term in some respects, is far from neutral to ecologists and evolution- ary biologists who expect adaptive claims to be cast within an ecological and evolu- tionary framework, and supported by research involving organisms representative of wild populations. Further controversy exists because there are many nutritionists who dispute the existence of adaptive DIT. Therefore, as long as there is a major and continuing dispute among scientists about DIT, it is important that terminology be neutral. The Case Against DIT There are a substantial number of studies that have failed to confirm the occurrence of DIT, and this has sparked a major debate among nutritionists. Research by Fuller (1983) and McCracken and McAllister (1984) failed to confirm the findings of Miller and Payne (1962) and Gurr et al. (1980) of DIT in the pig. Similarly, a variety of studies involving cafeteria feeding of rats have also reported negative results for DIT (McCracken 1975a; Tobin, Armitage, and Hervey 1981; Armitage et al. 1981; Barr and McCracken 1982; Bestley et al. 1982; McCracken and Barr 1982). DIT effected by a low-protein diet has also been questioned (McCracken 1975b), although in another study by McCracken (1976), an increase in energy expenditure and lower efficiency was shown for rats fed a low-protein diet. The controversy surrounding DIT has generated a series of highly polemical discussions in the literature (Hervey and Tobin 1981, and Rothwell and Stock 1981b; 19 Hervey and Tobin 1983, and Rothwell and Stock 1983; McCracken and Barr 1985, and Rothwell and Stock 1985a; Moore 1987, and Rothwell and Stock 1988). Clearly, the issue is far from resolution, and adopting a dogmatic, siege mentality has done little to enhance an open-minded discussion. However, this debate has stimulated energy balance research and it provides some of the impetus for my own research. DIT is a concept that can, and Should, be tested within an ecological framework, before it can be conclusively dismissed or wholly embraced. ECOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK FOR DIT RESEARCH DIT has been investigated in only a small number of species, most of which are bred specifically for laboratory research and can no longer be considered wild. The laboratory rat and mouse, both of which have been selectively bred within the stenothermal environment of a laboratory colony, are not necessarily fair representa- tives of natural populations of small mammals. Both rodents are from the same family, Muridae, and therefore are closely related. If we are to increase our knowledge about DIT, we Should make an attempt to broaden our understanding of the occurrence of this phenomenon in other species and families of organisms. If DIT is an adaptive process, we should be able to make predictions about its relevance to wild animals and then test them within an ecological framework. For instance, Rothwell and Stock (1985b) hypothesized that, since DIT is an adaptation to prevent obesity caused by excess food consumption, and NST is an adaptation to preserve homeothermy in the cold, then organisms living in environments where high temperatures and persistent food shortages are normal, may Show a complete absence 20 of thermogenic capacity. Rothwell and Stock suggested that some desert rodents (e.g. , spiny mouse and tuco tuco) may represent examples of this phenomenon, Since they quickly develop obesity when ample food is available (Rothwell and Stock 1985b). More research, however, is needed to substantiate these claims. Rothwell and Stock (1985c) used an ecologically-based argument to link DIT to the presence of BAT and the high capacity for thermogenesis observed in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Rothwell and Stock observed a 63 percent increase in oxygen consumption in marmosets injected with noradrenaline. They also found large BAT depots that showed a high degree of binding to GDP. Rothwell and Stock argued that, since marmosets evolved and normally live in a warm climate, it is unlikely that the high thermogenic capacity shown by marmosets is due to thermoregulatory NST. They suggested that, since marmosets primarily consume low-protein fruits, and since low-protein diets are known to activate DIT in rats, it is possible that the large BAT depots and high thermogenic capacity in marmosets are more related to their diet than to thermoregulatory demands. Rothwell and Stock proposed that DIT in marmosets could permit ingestion of large quantities of low- protein food while dissipating excess energy intake, thus resulting in ‘concentrating’ dietary protein (Rothwell and Stock 1985c). Because the use of wild animals in DIT research has been limited, more research of this type is needed. The full range of experimental techniques used to investigate DIT in rats and mice has yet to be implemented in the study of wild species. More studies of DIT involvement in different species are needed, and their results Should be examined within the context of each organism’s nutritional ecology 21 and thermal environment. An increase in the use of wild species will allow researchers to compare DIT responses (or parameters associated with DIT) between different species. This information can then be analyzed within the context of the ecology of each species, and researchers will be better able to observe any trends in responses that may be ecologically related. In this way, DIT responses viewed within the context of trophic levels, habitat quality, thermal conditions, or other ecological factors, can be compared for Similarities and differences. Without this type of research, our understanding of DIT, and its existence or relation to the ecology and evolution of different species, will remain minimal. My research focuses on DIT induced by low-protein intake and its relationship to the nutritional ecology of herbivores. If DIT evolved as an adaptive trait by natural selection, then an examination of the ecology of an organism exhibiting DIT could provide evidence for the selective pressures that led to the evolution of this trait. In the next chapter, an ecological framework is presented Showing that for many herbivores, high-quality food is a limiting resource in the environment. A lack of high-quality food can exert selective pressures that favor traits that enhance the ability to increase nutrient intake. For many herbivores, the quality of their food supply is determined primarily by the amount of metabolizable nitrogen it contains. The evidence presented in Chapter 11 suggests that a food supply low in nitrogen can have a negative influence on herbivore growth and fecundity, and therefore could be selective for traits such as DIT that may enhance the use of available nitrogen. Since the question of whether animal populations can be limited by resources is a perennial topic of controversy in 22 population and community ecology, it is discussed in depth. If animal populations cannot be limited by food resources, then it is unlikely that there would be sufficient selection pressure for the evolution of a trait or traits that enhance the use of food resources . CHAPTER II ECOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK Of the three mechanisms mentioned in the Introduction proposed by Himms-Hagen (1986) as possible functions for DIT, I chose the third proposal to investigate as a possible adaptive trait for a wild mammal. Himms-Hagen suggested that a small mammal could consume more energy than is required for daily maintenance, and DIT may function to optimize protein intake by providing a pathway through which excess energy or nonessential non-protein energy can be oxidized. An interesting feature of this hypothesis is that it implies that mammals have evolved a metabolic pathway to ‘waste’ energy. This notion is contrary to assumptions made for many optimality models of foraging strategy, in which it is presumed that animals function to minimize energy costs and maximize energy efficiency (Krebs and Davies 1987). However, some animals (e. g. , herbivores) may be limited more by the quality (nutrient content) of their diet than the quantity (energy content) (White 1978). Herbivores facing Shortages of high-quality nitrogenous foods in the environment may be forced to consume large quantities of low-protein food to maintain nitrogen balance. During growth and gestation, when protein synthesis is at its maximum, the ability to get enough nitrogen in the diet could be critical (Mattson 1980). The main hypothesis of my research, then, is that DIT is an intrinsic physio- logical response occurring in small, herbivorous mammals, which allows them to 23 24 increase nitrogen intake when dietary protein is low by oxidation of excess energy intake. The result is that essential dietary nitrogen is distilled from a bulky, low- nitrogen food supply. The following is a review of the research and discussion of the ecological ideas that are relevant to this hypothesis. THE HERBIVORE TROPHIC LEVEL If DIT evolved as an adaptive trait in herbivores to facilitate the maintenance of nitrogen balance, then resources consumed by herbivores must be limited in metabolizable nitrogen to exert selective pressure for the trait to evolve and be maintained. Evidence supporting the claim that nitrogen is a limiting resource for herbivores has been reviewed several times (McNeil and Southwood 197 8; White 1978; Mattson 1980). The genesis of much of this research can be traced back to ideas in population ecology concerning the regulation of the size of natural populations. One school of ecologists proposed that density-dependent factors (factors that are influenced by population size) such as predation and competition tend to stabilize populations at some equilibrium level (Nicholson 1933). Others argued that density-independent factors (factors that are independent of population size), such as those created by changing weather conditions, exert the greatest influence on population size by limiting population growth to periods of favorable environmental and climatic conditions (Andrewartha and Birch 1954). Many researchers who supported the idea of density-independence saw resources as limiting. They viewed the natural world as unpredictable and, at times, inhospitable, where highly variable extrinsic influences such as weather controlled 25 primary productivity and influenced herbivore food availability and population size. As a consequence, food abundance and population size for subsequent trophic levels in the food chain were also affected. This type of population regulation was termed “bottom up” regulation, since population size at all trophic levels was seen to be controlled by productivity at the lowest trophic level (plants) (White 1978). A different view presented by Hairston, Smith, and Slobodkin in a controver- sial paper written in 1960, outlined a set of ideas that became known as the HSS hypotheses. In brief, HSS proposed the following: 1. “Populations of producers, carnivores, and decomposers are limited by their respective resources in the classical density-dependent fashion. 2. lnterspecific competition must necessarily exist among the members of each of these three trophic levels. 3. Herbivores are seldom food-limited, appear most often to be predator- limited, and therefore are not likely to compete for common resourc- es. ” Their third point, that herbivores are seldom food-limited, is a conclusion based on the deductive premise that, since plants generally appear abundant, herbivores must be incapable of depleting them. Therefore, depletion of plants by herbivory is prevented by predators that limit the population size of herbivores to a level that has no appre- ciable impact on plant populations. The “top down” regulation hypothesis, where populations levels are assumed to be controlled by the influence of higher trophic levels on lower ones, was heavily criticized by Murdoch (1966), and Ehrlich and Birch (1967), who claimed that HSS failed to consider alternative hypotheses. They claimed that herbivore populations may be regulated by plants through a relative shortage of food brought on by a spatial 26 and temporal variability in plant distribution and quality. They also suggested that the evolution of spines and secondary plant compounds can render plants inedible and thus reduce resource availability. For the next twenty years, resource-based ideas of population ecology dominated research into plant-herbivore interactions, especially insect herbivore research. The HSS view continued to influence competition and predation theory. However, HSS failed as a whole to achieve dominance (Oksanen 1988). Recently, there has been a renewed interest in HSS. Oksanen et al. (1981) proposed that trophic structure is linked to physical factors and primary productivity (referred to as the CF model after Fretwell 1977 and Oksanen et al. 1981). Accord- ing to OF, as primary productivity increases, the length of the food chain increases. Food chains can vary in length continuously rather than discretely, depending on the number of organisms present in each trophic level. For instance, in a barren habitat, the food chain length is zero; as plants begin to grow, the food chain length increases incrementally. A food chain of length one is established with the appearance of the first herbivore. Food chain length two occurs when herbivores begin to impact the population size of the plant community. The food chain lengthens continuously to three when predators begin to influence the herbivore population; at food chain length four, a top—level predator regulates the population of the herbivore predator. In this model, HSS is viewed as depicting an ecosystem with a food chain length of three. However, in the OF model the herbivore population is now interacting dynamically with plant and predator populations, and the entire trophic structure is ultimately dependent on primary productivity. 27 The OF model predicts that in plant communities with low productivity, herbivore population size will be low and plant populations will be limited by the physical environment. As plant productivity increases, herbivore numbers will increase and eventually play a regulatory role in primary productivity. As the herbivore population increases, so does the carnivore population until a dynamic food chain with three trophic levels is established (Fretwell 1987; Oksanen 1988). Arditi, Ginzburg, and Akcakaya (1991) have criticized the OP model on the basis that it is too simplifying in its approach to the dynamics of predator-prey interactions. The Arditi et al. (1991) model of predator-prey interaction predicts equilibrium states where the densities of both predator and prey rise proportionately as primary productivity increases, which is counter to the OF model. While the OF model predicts that no two contiguous trophic levels will Show a Simultaneous increase, Arditi et al. cite numerous studies showing that herbivore and predator biomass covary with an increase in plant productivity. This view is supported by McNaughton et al. (1989), who reviewed 51 studies of a wide variety of ecosystems, including those with three or more trophic levels, and found that herbivore biomass increases with primary productivity. Interestingly, Moen and Oksanen (1991) reanalyzed the same data and came to a similar conclusion. The preceding discussion illustrates that although current ecological thought on trophic dynamics has not reached a consensus, it is still compatible with resource limitation theory. Furthermore, resource limitation theory is the only ecological theory that can account for the evolution of traits that are unique to herbivores. If plant resources are unlimited, as proposed by HSS, it is difficult to imagine how 28 selective pressure by predators can account for such obviously herbivorous traits as rumination, caecal digestion, or lophodont dentition. However, it is more likely that both plants and predators have shaped the evolutionary history of herbivores. For instance, the evolution of large body size in grazing ungulates may be a deterrent to predators as well as reduce the energetic cost of transport in migrational foraging. Similarly, the long neck of the giraffe may aid in visually locating predators and assist the giraffe in foraging on resources inaccessible to smaller herbivores. One of the basic assumptions of my research is that resource limitation is a major factor in the evolution of many herbivore traits. Charles Elton (1936) once wrote “Animals are not always struggling for existence, but when they do begin, they Spend the greater part of their lives eating. Feeding is such a universal and common- place business that we are inclined to forget its importance. The primary driving force of all animals is the necessity of finding the right type of food and enough of it. Food is the bunting question in animal society, and the whole structure and activities of the community are dependent upon questions of food-supply. ” Elton’s emphasis on “the right type of food” has been echoed by others (McNeil and Southwood 1978; White 1978; Mattson 1980) who claim that the right type of food means having enough metabolizable nitrogen in the diet to nourish the young and sustain growth. The following is a review and analysis of ideas and research related to relative food shortages in nature. 29 RELATNE FOOD SHORTAGE The concept of relative food shortage is important in understanding the selective pressures that can lead to the evolution of low-protein DIT. In this view, gross food energy is not a limiting resource for herbivores. As HSS pointed out, energy may be plentiful for herbivores. However, even if there is an abundant supply of energy available for herbivores, the nutrient quality of that food supply can change. Actual food quality2 of plants is highly variable and is dependent on a variety of factors such as seasonal change, ontogenetic stages, secondary plant chemicals, and plant-herbivore interactions. These factors, as well as others, can affect the spatial and temporal availability of a nutritionally-adequate food supply for a population of herbivores. Herbivores are often faced with what appears to be unlimited food; however, the quality of the food—especially the amount of metabolizable nitro- gen—may be so low that herbivores are unable to sustain normal growth and develop- ment. The effects can be especially devastating for young, growing animals that are highly dependent on dietary nitrogen to sustain development. Therefore, the interac- tion of herbivores with a food source that may vary temporally in nitrogen content, could involve major selective forces for traits that maximize nitrogen intake in what may be a nutritionally inadequate environment (White 1978). In an environment where energy is plentiful but the supply of nitrogen is variable, low-protein DIT could have evolved to aid in nitrogen intake during relative food shortages. Thus, this topic is covered in depth, since it is important to establish that a relative Shortage of high- 2High—quality food is defined here as plants high in usable protein and low in deleterious chemicals, after White (1978), and Bergeron and Jodoin (1987). 30 nitrogen food exists in the wild, if we are to believe that DIT evolved as a response to such a shortage. Change in Plant Quality and Its Influence on Herbivores TEMPORAL HETEROGENEITY The nitrogen content of different plant tissues can vary from 0.03 to 7.0 percent (0.2 to 44 percent protein)3 of dry weight, with seeds and new tissue growth having the highest levels. For most plants, the highest total nitrogen content occurs during seasonal periods of rapid growth. As the rate of plant growth declines over time, nitrogen concentrations are reduced and reach their lowest levels during senescence and abscission. Seasonal decline in nitrogen content can be as high as 80 percent of maximum (Mattson 1980). A good example illustrating seasonal changes in crude protein content in plants occurs in blue grama grass (Bouteloua gracilis), an important forage species in the shortgrass ecosystem of the Great Plains (Uresk and Sims 1975). Seasonal lows of 5 percent crude protein were recorded during March, when blue grama is dormant; crude protein increased to a high of 10 percent in June, during flowering, and progressively declined to 6 percent in December, when the grass again entered dormancy. Similar changes have been observed for broadleaf tree leaves (Feeny 1970; Ricklefs and Matthew 1982; Schultz, Nothnagle, and Baldwin 1982), mixed 3To convert nitrogen content to protein content, multiply nitrogen content by the gravimetric conversion factor 6.25, which is the one most commonly used. This Value can vary, depending on the type of protein or protein mixture being studied. For instance, the factor 6.38 is used to convert the nitrogen content of milk to protein Content (Helrich 1990). The chief protein in milk is the phosphoprotein casein. 31 forbs and grasses (Karasov 1985), the needles of conifers (Watt 1989) and phloem and xylem sap (Mittler 1953; Horsefield 1977; Barlow and Randolph 1978). Seasonal variation in nitrogen content in plants is partially a result of greater accumulation of cell wall material (lignin, cellulose, hemicellulose, and silica) compared to cell contents during growth, which dilutes the concentration of plant nitrogen; later, the translocation of nutrients out of the leaves during the onset of senescence causes a further reduction in nitrogen. The reduction of nitrogen content in the leaves of evergreen species, though, is primarily influenced by cell wall dilution, since evergreens minimize translocation of their nutrients, retaining them in their leaves during the winter (Chapin 1980). These processes are often driven by climatic changes in rainfall and the length of temperate growing seasons (Bell 1970; Phillipson 1975; Coe, Cumming, and Phillipson 1976). Freeze-thaw or wetting- drying cycles influence leaching and the breakdown of litter and organic compounds, which determine nutrient concentrations in soil solution. Soil nutrient availability affects nutrient absorption by plants, which eventually influences the nitrogen concentration in primary production (Chapin 1980). The impact of dry seasons on plant nitrogen and herbivores was examined by Sinclair (1974; 1975; 1979), who documented the relationship between African ungulates and the quality and quantity of available food in the East African Serengeti grasslands. Sinclair reported that during the dry season from July to October, the crude protein level for grasses dropped from eight percent to between one and three 32 percent4 (Sinclair 1975). Dry season crude protein was below the four to five percent level required to maintain adult body weight in African ungulates. Such low levels caused a loss in fat reserves, low efficiency of conversion of plant protein into animal protein (0.4 percent), and an increase in mortality rate during the dry season. Grazing time was similar during both the wet and dry seasons, indicating that ungulates were consuming the same amount of food throughout the year, but were unable to maintain nitrogen balance due to the low protein content of the food supply during the dry season (Sinclair 1974). As earlier mentioned, plant nitrogen content is strongly influenced by growth stage. New growth is high in protein but protein content declines rapidly with age (Uresk and Sims 1975; Mattson 1980; Jones and Wilson 1987; Watt 1987). In tropical rainforest trees, the young leaves and shoots highly favored by folivorous primates can contain more than 20 percent crude protein, while mature leaves have only between 7 and 15 percent crude protein (Hladik 1978). Newly flushed foliage of the Eucalyptus has a much higher nitrogen content than older growth; as nitrogen levels decrease with age, growth rates of leaf—eating larval beetles such as Paropsis atomaria and P. charybdis decline below maximum rates. P. atomaria larvae feed preferentially on new flush; however, when feeding on old growth, an inverse relationship is observed between foliage consumption by the larvae and leaf nitrogen ‘A 1 to 3 percent crude protein content is well below the 8 to 10 percent protein level required to invoke DIT in laboratory rats (Rothwell and Stock 1985c). Thus, it is likely that many herbivores typically consume diets in the wild that potentially could cause DIT. 33 content. Presumably, this feeding pattern optimizes nitrogen intake (Ohmart and Edwards 1991). The seasonal decline in the nitrogen content of plants is also correlated with a decrease in moisture content (Raupp and Denno 1983). In general, leaf water of terrestrial plants declines with maturity (Scriber and Slansky 1981). Reduction in plant moisture content may reduce the suitability of plants as a food source. Low water content in forage may cause water stress in herbivores and reduce their ability to use plant nutrients. Scriber (1977) found that, when compared to controls, the efficiency of nitrogen assimilation and both gross and net energy efficiencies declines in lepidopteran larvae fed wild cherry leaves with low moisture content. Reese and Beck (1978) noted a similar phenomenon occurred in the black cutworm (Agrotis ipsilon), when larvae were fed artificial diets of varying moisture content. The physiological efficiency model proposed by Scriber (1984a; 1984b) links host-plant suitability for insect herbivores with leaf water-nitrogen composition, and predicts optimal host-plant utilization when leaf water and nitrogen content are at their highest concentrations. The model is supported by evidence showing that geographical variation in larval growth rates of the silkmoth Callosamia promethea is largely attributable to variations in leaf water and nitrogen content of host plants (Scriber 1984b). ’This response is similar to the DIT response observed in vertebrates in the lab. As the amount of nitrogen assimilated from the diet declines, net energy efficiency also declines. 34 Herbivore Feeding Habits Seasonal shifts in food quality are often reflected in the feeding habits of wild animals. Lindroth and Batzli (1984a) observed that meadow voles (Microtus pennsyl- vanicus) fed on the green shoots of monocots and dicots when available, then switched to seeds, roots, and grasses during autumn and winter. Plants that were preferred forage during the summer (Tnfolium pratense and Taraxacum ofi‘icinale) had higher levels of crude protein (20. 8 and 11.0 percent, respectively) than less- preferred species such as Poa pratensis and Andropogon gerardii (6.3 and 3. 8 percent, respectively). Karasov (1982) found a similar pattern for omnivorous antelope ground squirrels (Ammospermophilus leucurus). In the spring, the dominant component of the natural diet of Mojave Desert antelope ground squirrels is moist, high-nitrogen vegetation that is easily digested. During midsummer, when arthropod populations are peaking, arthropods make up a considerable portion of the squirrels’ diet. By late summer and fall, nitrogen levels and dry matter digestibility of vegetation decline, and seeds and arthropods dominate the diet. After the winter rains, ground squirrels readily feed on new plant growth, and md consumption declines. During feeding trials, Karasov found that ground squirrels maintained constant body weights while consuming vegetation collected during the spring, but lost weight on a diet of late-summer vegetation. When the latter diet was supplement- ed with seeds and arthropods, weight constancy was regained (Karasov 1982). Preferential fwding by herbivores resulting from phenological or ontogenetic variability in food quality has been observed in many different animal groups. These include the impala (Rodgers 1976), voles (Goldberg, Tabroff, and Tamarin 1980; 35 Bergeron and Jodoin 1987), deer (Klein 1970), Icelandic ptarmigan (Gardarsson and Moss 1970), pocket gophers (Gettinger 1984) and many Species of insects (see Denno and McClure 1983, and references therein). Thus, the supply of high quality food for herbivores, influenced by climate and growth stage, varies over time and can affect feeding patterns and survivorship of herbivores. According to Sinclair (1975), the quality of the food supply for a herbivore, determined by its crude protein content, can become so low due to seasonal variability that the herbivore can no longer maintain its body weight, even through sustained grazing. In African ungulates, selective feeding may be beneficial at times, but many species are forced into opportunistic nomadism, moving to regions where rainfall permits an adequate food supply (McNaughton 1987). During periods of sustained drought, survivorship declines (Sinclair 1975). Thus, the effects of climate can create conditions whereby herbivores are faced with a food supply that is too low in usable protein to sustain normal body weight or permit growth. Under these conditions, traits that enhance protein uptake should be favored by natural selection. If DIT enables a herbivore to increase nitrogen uptake under harsh conditions such as this, then it is likely that it could have evolved by natural selection as a physiological response to low ambient protein levels in food resources. SECONDARY PLANT CHEMICALS Plants produce a variety of allelochemicals that strongly influence the nutri- tional ecology of herbivores. Plant secondary chemicals can alter the nutritional Status of herbivores by functioning as toxins, feeding deterrents, or digestibility 36 reducers. Furthermore, they can have indirect effects on herbivore nutrition by altering intestinal microflora or adversely affecting the herbivore’s biochemical detoxication systems (Lindroth 1988). Ultimately, plant allelochenricals can affect the feeding habits, digestion and conversion efficiencies, health status, and survivorship of herbivores (Scriber and Slansky 1981; Lindroth and Batzli 1984b; Lindroth, Batzli, and Avildsen 1986; Bergeron and Jodoin 1987; Bryant et a1. 1991). Plant Defense Theory Plant secondary chemicals were not always recognized by scientists as having antiherbivore properties. Before Gottfried Fraenkel developed the theory in the 1950s that plant secondary chemicals defended plants from herbivore attack, they were viewed primarily as irrelevant, biologically meaningless, waste by-products of plant metabolism, occasionally useful only to humans for medicinal purposes (Grant 1984). Since then, considerable research has been done in an attempt to fit allelochenricals into a general theory of plant antiherbivore chemistry. In the mid 1970s, the theory of plant apparency was developed by Feeny (1976) and Rhoades and Cates (1976) to explain observed herbivore feeding patterns (e. g. , Feeny 1970, and Tahvanainen and Root 1972). The plant apparency theory provided an evolutionary framework for studies on the coevolutionary interactions between plants and herbivores, and is based on the idea that production of secondary compounds imposes a metabolic cost to the plant. Apparency theory holds that the allelochemical profile of a plant is primarily determined by how easily it is found (how “apparent” it is) by a herbivore. Large and/or long-lived plants easily found by herbivores are predicted to defend themselves 37 by investing a large amount of primary production into “quantitative” chemicals, such as polyphenols (tannins) or resins, which function in a dose-dependent fashion to deter herbivory. In contrast, small plants and/or ephemerals (“unapparent” plants) should rely on “qualitative” chemicals such as toxins, which suffice in small amounts to deter most herbivores and, therefore, require less energy expenditure to produce (Howe and Westley 1988). Because apparent plants are easily found, quantitative chenrical defenses are predicted to be effective against all herbivores. In contrast, Specialist herbivores can easily neutralize qualitative defense toxins by employing enzyme defenses that presumably have evolved through long-term interactions with plants. Production of qualitative low-cost toxins is considered an effective defense for short-lived and scarce plants because herbivores have minimal contact time and are therefore less likely to evolve the means to detoxify them (Howe and Westley 1988). To explain the heterogeneous distribution of secondary compounds within a plant, and preferential feeding by herbivores on different plant parts, McKey (1979) extended the principle of apparency to plant components. According to McKey, older woody tissue and mature leaves should rely heavily on quantitative chemical defense, while new growth and seeds are more likely to contain qualitative defensive chemi- cals, such as toxins. The theory of plant apparency is supported by numerous studies of plant- herbivore interactions (Feeny 1970; Root 1973; Fee'ny 1976; Futuyma 1976; Cates and Rhoades 1977; Ikeda, Matsumura, and Benjamin 1977; Slansky and Feeny 1977; Cates 1980; Berenbaum 1981; Henderson 1990). However, in recent years it has 38 become clear that plant apparency cannot fully explain all patterns of allelochemic defense, and there has been considerable revision in defense theory to accommodate new insights. Most notable are the ideas of Bryant, Chapin, and Klein (1983) and Coley, Bryant, and Chapin (1985), who propose that resource availability is the evolutionary determinant that controls the carbon/ nutrient balance of plants. According to these researchers, plant resources ultimately control the amount of energy than can be allocated to plant defense and the nature of the defense. In this view, when resources are limited, slow-growing plants are favored by natural selection. These plants invest heavily in constitutive chemical defenses because they are unable to acquire new resources to compensate for tissue lost to heavy browsing (e.g., boreal forest trees). In contrast, plants that are not limited by available resources have high growth rates, allocate less energy to chemical defense, and respond to excess grazing through compensatory growth. Grarninoids that retain large nutrient reserves underground are good examples. The theoretical framework interpreting the ecological role of allelochenrics continues to grow and change as our knowledge of plant-herbivore interactions and plant secondary chemistry increases. Current theories on mammal-plant chemical ecology and evolutionary relationships can be found in Palo and Robbins 1991 (and references therein); for insects, see Barbosa and Letoumeau 1988 (and references therein). Defense theory has provided and continues to provide an ecological and evolutionary framework for research on plant-herbivore interactions. One of its major accomplishments is that it has cast a new light on the green world; plants are no longer viewed as passive participants in the ecology of herbivores. According to 39 Janzen ( 1978): “The plant world is not colored green; it is colored morphine, caffeine, tannin, phenol, terpene, canavarrine, latex, phytohaemagglutinin, oxalic acid, saponin, L-dopa, etc.” Thus, our increasing knowledge about plant secondary compounds adds a dynamic facet to the study of herbivore-plant interactions that was previously unknown or ignored. Defense theory also serves to illustrate that herbivores can appear to have a plentiful food supply, but the quality of their food resources can be limited. For herbivores, food Shortages are not always readily apparent. Impact of Chemical Defense on Herbivores The quality and quantity of available food for herbivores can be altered by plant secondary compounds. This, in turn, can lead to decreased herbivore fitness through increased mortality, reduced growth rate, or lowered fecundity (Rhoades and Cates 1976). Also noteworthy is that the impact of plant chenricals on the nutritional status of a herbivore is influenced by a variety of intrinsic herbivore characteristics, including the animal’s age, sex, genetic makeup, and prior feeding experience with various allelochemics (Lindroth 1988). Ultimately, actual food quality depends on the interaction of both plant and herbivore traits. For instance, the arginine analog L- canavanine, found in the seeds of the neotropical legume Dioclea megacarpa, can be highly toxic to many herbivores (e.g., the silkworm, boll weevil, and fruit fly). However, some specialist herbivores, such as the bruchid beetle (Caryedes brasiliensis), not only have circumvented the toxic effects of L-canavanine, but use it to generate dietary nitrogen (Rosenthal and Bell 1979). 40 Thus, broad generalizations about the effects of specific allelochemicals (or even a particular class of chemicals, such as tannins) are difficult to make because of the dynamic interactions between plants and herbivores. Moreover, our knowledge about allelochemic-herbivore interaction is still growing; as the mass of information increases, so does our awareness of the increased complexity of interaction. There- fore, while generalizations are inevitable, they are not intended herein to be dogmatic. Texins mg ffling deterrents. Toxic allelochemics (TA) are plant chemicals that, in small dosages, can cause physical damage, sickness, or death in a herbivore (Grant 1984). They are widely distributed among plants; for instance, the toxin hydrogen cyanide has been found in over one thousand species (Bell 1978). Alkaloids, terpenoids, toxic amino acids, phytohemaglutinins, toxic lipids, glucosinol- ates, proteinase inhibitors, saponins, and cyanogenic glycosides are some of the known types of TAs (Bell 1978; Scriber 1984a; Bemays, Driver, and Bilgener 1989). Their effects on herbivores include severe convulsions, organ damage, hair loss, tumor formation, weight loss, abortion, fetal damage, growth retardation, inhibition of sexual maturation, neurological damage, reduction in life span, and death (Freeland and Janzen 1974). For example, many field and laboratory studies on voles suggest that plant secondary compounds can affect their health and survivorship (J ung and Batzli 1981; Lindroth and Batzli 1984b; Bergeron, Jodoin, and Jean 1987; Lindroth, Batzli, and Avildsen 1986; Bergeron and Jodoin 1987; 1989). Because of the extreme detrimental effects and wide dissenrination of TAs, Freeland and Janzen (1974) suggested that some herbivores use behavioral mecha- nisms associated with sensory capabilities and memory to avoid exceptionally toxic 41 plants or plant parts. Learned aversions and preferences are well known for laborato- ry rats that develop meal-induced food preferences from single-trial feeding (Lindroth 1988). Vaughan and Czaplewski (1985) proposed that young Stephens’ woodrats (Neotoma stephensz) learn from their mother to avoid juniper forage high in tannins and terpenoids, through dietary training during a long preweaning period. Many studies have shown that food preferences are often negatively correlated with plant toxin levels. Feeding on bracken fern (Pteridium aquilinum) by sheep, deer, and locusts, is strongly inhibited during seasonal peaks in cyanogenesis (Coo- per-Driver, Finch, and Swain 1977). Evidence from experimental feeding trials indicates that some animals will voluntarily reduce food intake to well below mainte- nance level when fed either browse containing high levels of allelochemics or laboratory food treated with browse extracts (Bryant et al. 1991). Similarly, feeding deterrence caused by plant allelocherrrics has been observed in meadow voles (Kendall and Sherwood 1975; Kendall and Leath 1976), mule deer (Schwartz, Regelin, and Nagy 1980), tundra voles and lemmings (Jung and Batzli 1981), Abert squirrels (Zhang and States 1991), snowshoe hares (Bryant 1981), and many other wild animals. AS previously mentioned, young, growing plant tissue is usually higher in nitrogen than older tissue, and senescence routinely results in a seasonal decline in nitrogen. Although new plant growth may be high in nitrogen content, it often contains elevated levels of TAs that can function as feeding deterrents (McKey 1979). Bryant’s study (1981) of snowshoe hares (Lepus americanus) browsing on boreal forest trees showed that hares were deterred from feeding on adventitious shoots high 42 in terpenes and phenolic resins. Hares preferred, instead, to feed on more mature twigs, even though the adventitious shoots were higher in nitrogen content. Comparable results were found for snowshoe hares feeding on Alaska feltleaf willow (Salix alaxensis) (Bryant et al. 1985). Freeland and Janzen (1974) suggested that feeding strategies of herbivores depend on both specific plant defenses and the herbivore’s nutritient requirements. According to Freeland and Janzen ( 1974), generalist herbivores sample a variety of foods and depend on learning mechanisms, in response to adverse internal physiologi- cal effects induced by phytochemicals, to select a diet that will maximize nutrient intake and minimize the effects of TAs. Thus, diet diversification results in the intake of a variety of toxins, but limits the dosage of any one toxin. These ideas on food sampling and diet diversification are now being incorporated into optimality models of herbivore foraging strategies. For instance, Belovsky and Schnritz (1991) developed a linear programming model of optimal foraging for herbivores that incorporates phytochemical constraints to diet selection. Research by Lindroth and Batzli (1984a) and Lindroth et al. (1986) suggests that both the allelochenricals and the protein content in plants can influence foraging preferences in M. penmylvanicus in bluegrass and prairie habitats. Preferred foods such as Trzfolium pratense and Taraxacum ofiicinale not only had high levels of protein (previously discussed), they also had high levels of phenolics. Poa pratensis had low levels of both crude protein and phenolics and was least preferred by voles. Andropogon gerardii was also avoided; A. gerardii had low protein content and intermediate levels of phenolics. Voles showed only a moderate preference for 43 feeding on Lespedeza cuneata, even though crude protein content was relatively high. L. cuneata had the highest concentration of phenolics of all the plants tested, suggest- ing that, although protein content was high, it was not high enough to overcome the deterrent effects of phenolics and stimulate feeding. Lindroth and Batzli (1984b) also tested the effects of the interaction of allelo- chemics and protein on the growth rate of prairie voles (M. ochrogaster). Their results showed that the plant chemicals quercetin and tannic acid reduced growth rates in voles when protein levels were low, but had no effect at higher protein levels. Thus, at least in some animals, if the nitrogen level of foodstuffs is high enough, the detrimental effects of some TAs can be reduced or eliminated. However, there may be a point at which TA levels or potency are so high that feeding is prevented, regardless of the plant protein content. Another plant chemical, quebracho, was highly unpalatable and, as a result, was lethal at all protein levels (Lindroth and Batzli 1984b). Feeding studies by Bergeron and Jodoin (1987) and Bucyanayandi and Bergeron (1990) found that, in an old-field community, M. pennsylvanicus preferred food that was high in protein and low in phenolics. Contrary to many optimality models that use energy as the optimal nutritional factor, voles did not appear to be energy limited or choose food based on energy content. Food selection was based on proteinzphenolic ratios. Therefore, Bergeron and Jodoin ( 1987) defined “high quality” food for voles as “plant species that contain the highest level of protein with the lowest levels of total phenolics. ” 44 Plant toxins, then, can linrit a herbivore’s intake of high-nitrogen food by reducing palatability of foodstuffs that are high in nitrogen content. Many herbivores may be forced to feed on a variety of plants, including those with low nitrogen content, to limit the intake of any particular toxin, and thus avoid injurious toxic effects. Moreover, intake of food with high protein content may negate some deleterious effects of toxins. Herbivores that choose to fwd on foods that are low in protein because they are also low in allelochenric content, could increase consumption of low-protein forage to increase bulk protein intake. Under these conditions, low-protein DIT could function to eliminate excess caloric intake and enhance protein intake. W. The distinction between plant toxins and allelochenrics that reduce digestive efficiency is not always a clear one (Meyer and Karasov 1989). For instance, tannins can function to limit the absorption of nutrients across the gut wall (Palo 1985). Also, they can be toxic, causing erosion of the intestinal mucosa or lesions in the liver or kidneys (Bemays et al. 1989). Meyer and Karasov (1989) examined the impact of tannins on five species of rodents and two species of lago- morphs and observed that tannins reduced plant digestibility. They also noted that there was some evidence of postdigestive toxic effects (animals were deterred from feeding and lost weight, even when there was no significant effect on digestive efficiency), although this was not measured directly. In general, though, digestibility- reducing allelochenrics (DRA) are functionally different from toxins. Whereas toxin effects tend to be qualitative and act directly on herbivore tissue, the effects of DRAS 45 are usually quantitative, dosage dependent, and more indirect (Cates and Rhoades 1977). Tannins. Tannins are polymeric phenolic compounds, synthesized primarily from products of the shikimic acid pathway in plants that can act as complexing agents capable of binding with nutrients, especially proteins. In addition to the aforementioned toxic effects, tannins can also hinder plant digestion in herbivores by fornring precipitates with dietary proteins, amino acids, fats, nucleic acids, polysac- charides, or digestive enzymes (Bemays et al. 1989). Also, tannins may inhibit protein absorption in the intestinal mucosa (Mole et al. 1990b). Tannins have been shown to reduce both protein and dry-matter digestibility in runrinants (Barry, Manley, and Duncan 1986; Robbins et al. 1987; Robbins et al. 1991). The relative role tannins play as DRAS is a topic of current debate (Bemays et al. 1989). Nevertheless, fecal nitrogen data show that ingestion of tannins by herbi- vores results in significant nitrogen losses (Glick and Joslyn 1970; Lindroth et al. 1986). A study by Hanley et al. (1992) showed that, for cervids in the genus Odocoileus feeding on fireweed (Epilobium angustzfoliwn), there was a 44 percent reduction in digestible protein due to the effects of tannins. Mole, Butler, and Iason (1990a) suggested that the primary source for tannin- induced excretory nitrogen losses is endogenous, rather than dietary, proteins. Secretion by herbivores of proline-rich salivary proteins, which have a high affinity for tannins, has been proposed as a defensive mechanism against the negative effects of dietary tannins (Mole et al. 1990a). High-affinity, tannin-binding salivary proteins conceivably could lower tannin absorption and concomitant toxicity. Binding of 46 tannins by salivary proteins could reduce the amount of dietary proteins bound by tannins, and also minimize the loss of digestive enzymes due to tannin binding. Because proline-rich salivary proteins have a higher affinity for tannins than dietary proteins (more tannin is bound per unit of salivary protein than dietary protein), the herbivore experiences less protein loss than if all the tannins were bound by dietary proteins alone (Robbins et al. 1991). Other sources of non—dietary fecal nitrogen besides salivary proteins include mucus and cells sloughed off from the gut as a result of tannin abrasion (Freeland, Calcott, and Anderson 1985). Regardless of the source of increases in fecal nitrogen, the net effect of dietary tannins on herbivores is a decrease in the efficiency of nitrogen retention, and a negative impact on nitrogen balance. Lignins. Lignins are another group of plant phenolic polymers produced biosynthetically from products of the shikimic acid pathway (Swain 1979). Within plants, lignins bind to cellulose or hemicellulose in the cell walls, imparting a structural rigidity or “woodiness” to the plant. According to Swain (1979), “Wood, as Gertrude Stein nright have said, is wood and, one might add, the quintessence of woodiness lies in the presence of lignins.” Lignins also stiffen and toughen the stems and leaves of nonwoody plant tissues (Howe and Westley 1988). Lignins are the primary dietary component of forage that limits digestibility (Van Soest 1982). Few organisms are capable of digesting lignins; therefore, lignins binding to plant cell walls reduce the digestibility of cell wall carbohydrates (Howe and Westley 1988). It has been suggested that lignins reduce both dietary carbohydrate and protein digestion by binding in vivo to these nutrients (Swain 1979). 47 Another hypothesis is that they can limit digestible protein by entrapment of nutrients within cell walls (Bell 1971). However, there is little evidence available to support either of these claims (Parra 1978; Van Soest 1982; Buchsbaum, Wilson, and Valiela 1986). There is some evidence, though, that lignins bind amino acids within grasses and legumes, thus reducing nitrogen availability (Van Soest 1982). Lignins do play a role in reducing nitrogen availability in forage by dilution effects. The concentration of lignins in plant cells can reach 40 percent or more (Swain 1979). A decline in protein content and a concomitant increase in lignins is often associated with increasing plant maturity. Daylength and temperature are key factors determining structural changes in plants as they mature. For example, as temperature increases, so does plant metabolism, resulting in an increase in the rate of production of plant structural components (lignins, cellulose, and hemicellulose) and a decline in cytoplasmic metabolites. Thickening of the cell wall by structural growth and lignification leads to a decline in cytoplasm volume by increasing the limitation set by the physical dimensions of the cell wall on intracellular volume. Because cytoplasmic volume is limited, the amount of intracellular nutrients that can accumulate is also limited (Van Soest 1982; Schwartz and Hobbs 1985). Cellulose and hemicellulose. Along with lignin, cellulose and hemicellulose comprise what is commonly called “dietary fiber” (Milton 1979). Cellulose and henricellulose are complex polysaccharides that form the carbohydrate portion of plant cell walls. These structural carbohydrates cannot be broken down by the digestive enzymes of most herbivores, especially vertebrates. Digestion of the carbohydrate portion of fiber in many herbivores requires symbiotic gut 'microbes that have 48 cellulolytic enzymes capable of catalyzing fiber breakdown. In vertebrates, this process is enhanced by fermentation, which usually occurs in a special fermentation chamber formed by modification of the digestive tract. The chamber usually occurs as an enlarged foregut compartment typical of ruminants and also found in macropods and colobid monkeys. It also may exist as a modified hindgut sac (enlarged colon or caecum) found in perissodactyls and microtines, as well as other animals (Milton 1979). Because structural carbohydrates are difficult to digest, the presence of cellulose and henricellulose in plants reduces their digestibility. However, like lignins, cellulose and hemicellulose have only a moderate affect on the digestibility of cell contents (Van Soest 1982; Buchsbaum et a1. 1986). Structural carbohydrates do affect food nutrients and nitrogen availability, though, by dilution effects similar to those previously described for lignins. Silica. Silica is an oxide of Silicon, and is commonly metabolized by plants and incorporated into cell walls as a structural component. Silica is completely indigestible by herbivores (Howe and Westley 1988), and like fiber, it reduces food quality through adulteration of nutrients. Digestibility of foodstuffs can be reduced by silica through the abrasive effects of its crystals that accelerate herbivore tooth wear (McNaughton et al. 1985). Worn teeth limit a herbivore’s ability to masticate food before it is passes into the gut. A reduction in mastication reduces the surface area of chewed food that would be available for enzymatic digestion in the gut, thus reducing forage digestibility. 49 Silica can also be toxic to herbivores. In grazing ungulates, it is known to cause silica urolithiasis (a fatal urinary tract disease) and augment the formation of esophageal cancer (McNaughton et al. 1985). Silica is also a well-known feeding deterrent due to the sharp siliceous protrusions on leaf fringes. Although its occur- rence in grazing plants is dependent on soil type and availability of silica in the soil (Van Soest 1968), feeding pressure can also be a factor. In heavily grazed areas of the Serengeti, the concentration of silica in plants is much greater than in adjacent areas with less grazing pressure. This pattern persists when transplanted plants from different grazing areas are reared in the laboratory, showing genetic differences presumably resulting from natural selection due to grazing pressure (McNaughton et al. 1985). STRUCTURAL DEFENSE The idea that plants actively discourage herbivore feeding and therefore limit food availability is most evident when the array of external mechanical defenses is examined. The so-called “barbed wire syndrome” is exemplified by the sharp spines of cacti, which are well-known feeding deterrents against large, mammalian herbi- vores (Cooper and Owen-Smith 1986). Spinescence in holly trees is known to dis- courage leaf-eating caterpillars, and specialized hairs called trichomes create a dense, tangled barrier to many feeding insects and are even known to pierce their antagonists (Grant 1984). Some hairs are glandular in nature; they secrete repellant oils or sticky exudates that can entrap invertebrate herbivores. Stinging nettles have trichomes that secrete an irritating chemical that can function as a feeding deterrent (Grant 1984). Thistles are well-armored against herbivores due to the extensive use of trichomes, 50 and are often highly conspicuous compared to other plants in heavily grazed pastures (Grant 1984). Agriculturalists, well aware of the effectiveness of structural defenses in plants, select genetic strains of cotton or beans that are highly pubescent to discourage the attack of small insects and mites (Howe and Westley 1988). Fiber is also part of a plant’s structural defense, in that it increases leaf toughness, making a plant more difficult for herbivores to chew or penetrate (Tabashnik and Slansky 1987; Ohmart and Edwards 1991). Leaf toughness can cause decreased food consumption, reduced growth, and low survival rates in foliage- chewing insects (Tabashnik and Slansky 1987). INDUCED DEFENSE Plant defense in response to herbivory is commonly thought to occur within a chronic time frame by coevolutionary changes (or by what Fox (1981) calls “diffuse coevolution”) (see Spencer 1988, and references therein). However, some plants are capable of mobilizing defense mechanisms within an acute time frame. This type of response by plants is called induced defense (Rhoades 1985), and it entails a phenotypic change in plants in response to herbivory that is protective for the plants and usually deleterious to herbivores (Myers 1988). For example, Benz (1974; 1977, as cited by Rhoades 1983) found that defoliation of larch (Larix decidua) by the larch budmoth (Zeirapherra diniana) resulted in phenotypic changes in new tree growth. Fresh growth by Larix occurring the year after defoliation consisted of smaller needles higher in fiber and lower in nitrogen content than needles from the previous year. This new growth may sometimes be covered with an exudate of oleoresin that was not present the year 51 before. For the budmoth, these changes caused reduced palatability of larch needles, low assimilation efficiencies, high mortality, and reduced fecundity. Bryant (1981) found that some species of boreal forest trees respond to severe browsing by snowshoe hares (L. americanus) by production of adventitious shoots that have significantly higher concentrations of terpene and phenolic resins than normally found in mature growth. Bryant (1981) also found that hares avoided mature-growth- l form twigs when they were coated with adventitious root resins. Tahvanainen et al. (1985) noted that an acute production of defensive allelochemicals occurred when mountain hares (L. timidus) fed on northern willow (Salix spp.) during the winter in the northern boreal zone. Snowshoe hares feeding on Alaska paper birch and feltleaf willow also induced an acute, chemically-mediated defensive response (Reichardt et al. 1984; Bryant et al. 1985). Lindroth and Batzli ( 1986) found that phenolic concentrations increased in old field alfalfa under heavy grazing pressure during vole population peaks. Oksanen and Oksanen (1981, as cited by Lindroth 1989) observed a nearly twofold rise in phenol/nitrogen ratios in bilberry (Vaccinium mym’llus) following intense grazing by cycling populations of lemmings. Other plants known to exhibit induced chemical defense include brown algae (Fucus distichus) (Van Alstyne 1988), birch (Betula pubescens) (Hanhimaeki 1989), lodgepole pine (Pinus contarta) (Leather, Watt, and Forrest 1987), cotton (Karban 1987), soybeans (Chiang et al. 1987; Lin, Kogan, and Fischer 1990), wild parsnip (Pastinaca sativa) (Zangerl and Berenbaum 1990), pondwwd (Potamogeton colorants) (J effries 1990) , and quaking aspen (Popular tremuloides) (Clausen et al. 1989). 52 Apparently, mechanical defenses of plants can also be induced by herbivory. When browsed by goats, Acacia depranolobium responded by producing longer thorns (Young 1987). Young (1987) noted that thorns on browsed branches were longer than those on unbrowsed branches, both within individual trees and between browsed and unbrowsed trees. Similar results were noted for trees browsed by giraffes (Milewski, Young, and Madden 1991). SUMMARY It is clear from the proceeding discussion that herbivore food resources can be highly variable in terms of their nutrient content. This may be especially true of the nitrogen content of forage, which can vary temporally, becoming so low that the food supply may no longer be adequate to sustain growth or survival. Secondary plant chemicals and structural defenses can also reduce a herbivore’s ability to obtain high- quality, high-nitrogen food, by deterrent effects that force herbivores to feed on less nutritious plants and plant parts (plants and plant parts lower in nitrogen). Plant chemical compounds can also reduce nitrogen assimilation by binding with dietary proteins that form complexes that are excreted. Since nitrogen can be a limiting nutrient for herbivores, it is likely that natural selection will favor traits that enhance nitrogen uptake. If herbivores are forced to consume more food when nitrogen levels are low, a physiological response such as low-protein DIT could be adaptive by augmenting nitrogen assimilation. 53 Herbivore Response to a Variable Food Supply INFLUENCE OF SUPPLEMENTAL FOOD To assess the impact of food availability on vertebrate herbivore population dynamics, a number of food supplementation studies have been conducted. The results of these studies have been far from unequivocal, and controversy continues over the role that food plays in demographic patterns (Boutin 1990). Most of the debate centers around the question of whether predation or food shortage regulates population cycles in small mammals such as hares and microtine rodents. The issue of population cycles will not be addressed here, but some patterns that result from supplemental feeding of herbivores are worth mentioning and point to the importance of the availability of high-quality food in herbivore ecology. Reproduction An escalation in breeding activity often occurs in response to supplemental feeding (Boutin 1990). (See Table 2.1 for a compilation of studies focusing on changes in reproductive activity in herbivores due to supplemental feeding.) Andrze- jewski (1975) noted a significant increase in winter breeding by the bank vole, Clethrianomys glareolus, after supplemental feeding with oats. According to Andrze- jewski (1975), in food-supplemented populations, 50 percent of the individuals found in the spring were born during the winter, indicating extensive winter breeding. This contributed to a two-year post-winter population increase that resulted in a population Size that was two to four times bigger than any population of the previous six years; control-plot animals did not increase. An early onset of the breeding season was also observed (Table 2.1). Massive reproductive activity began in January instead of at 54 Table 2.1. Influence of supplemental food on reproduction in hares and microtines (+, increase; 0, no change; blank, not examined). Adapted from Boutin 1990. Brwding Season Species length Intensity Reference Hares Lepus americanus + Windberg and Keith (1976) + + Vaughan and Keith (1981) + Boutin (1984) 0 Krebs et al. (1986) 0 Krebs, Boutin, and Gilbert (1986) Microtines Clethrionomys glareolus + + Andrzejewski (1975) Clethrionomys rutilus + Gilbert and Krebs (1981) Microtus califamicus + + Ford and Pitelka (1984) Microtus ochrogaster + Cole and Batzli (1978) + Desy and Batzli (1989) Microtus pennsylvanicus + Desy and Thompson (1983) Microtus townsendii + 0 Taitt and Krebs (1981) + + Taitt and Krebs (1983) the end of March or the beginning of April, which is usually regarded as the begin- ning of the reproductive season (Andrzejewski 1975). Food addition can cause an increase in breeding intensity. Cole and Batzli (1978) found that the proportion of reproductive female prairie voles in a population fed supplementally with rabbit pellets was Significantly greater than in a control population. An increase in the proportion of scrotal males in the experimental group 55 also was noted. Table 2.1 lists Studies that have shown increases in breeding intensity (i.e. , the proportion of females breeding) that occurred as a result of added food. Also of interest, Negus and Berger (1977) found that feeding limited supple- ments of fresh green wheatgrass can trigger the onset of reproductive activity in a nonbreeding winter population of M. montanus. All of the females in an experimental group became pregnant while controls remained in nonbreeding condition. According to Negus and Berger (1977), reproductive activity is cued by chemical signals in the plant food resources. This suggests that some herbivores may depend on temporal changes in food quality to signal reproductive behavior. Growth Many studies have shown that supplemental fwding of small herbivores in the wild can have a positive influence on their growth. Increases in peak body weight and the rate of growth have been recorded for hares and voles receiving food supplementation (Table 2.2). Both adult and juvenile animals Show higher peak weights when fed supplemental food, compared to control animals. Desy and Batzli (1989) observed an increase in the rate of growth of young prairie voles (weight < 25 g when first captured) on supplemental feeding regimes. Mean adult body weight of voles was also greater in food-supplemented populations. The importance of growth rate and body size is underscored by the fact that they are frequently used qualitatively to assess fitness. Reproductive maturity in many mammals is often determined by an animal’s size instead of its age (White 1983; Rayor 1985). 56 Table 2.2. Influence of supplemental food on growth in hares and microtines (+ , increase; 0, no change; blank, not examined). Adapted from Boutin 1990. Body Growth Species weight rate Reference Hares Windberg and Keith ( 1976) + Vaughan and Keith (1981) Boutin ( 1984) + Krebs et al. (1986) Krebs, Boutin, and Gilbert (1986) + Smith et al. (1988) Microtines E L. americanus +o++++ C. glareolus Andrzejewski (1975) Banach (1986) C. rutilus 0 Gilbert and Krebs (1981) + Krebs and DeLong (1965) Cole and Batzli (1978) + Desy and Batzli (1989) + Desy and Thompson (1983) + Taitt and Krebs (1981) Taitt and Krebs (1983) M. californicus M. ochrogaster M. pennsylvanicus M. townsendii +++++oo++ FORAGE QUALITY Herbivore performance (survival, growth, and reproduction) is usually enhanced in habitats with high-quality forage (Lindroth 1989). Cole and Batzli (1979) found that prairie voles in an alfalfa habitat had higher fecundity, survival, and body weights than those in an adjacent bluegrass habitat. Levels of digestible energy, crude protein, calcium, phosphorous, and sodium were higher for forage in the alfalfa 5 7 habitat. White (1983) compared the growth rates of reindeer (Rangifer tarandus) that grazed on low-quality forage ranges (dwarf birch/sedge) with those that fed on ranges with high-quality forage (willow/ sedge). Fawn growth rates were higher on the willow-sedge ranges; milk production was reduced for reindeer on the low-quality ranges, which presumably led to low fawn growth rates. COMPENSATORY MECHANISMS As noted previously, wild herbivores must contend with a nutrient supply that can be highly variable in both quality and quantity. Plant tissues can fluctuate greatly in fiber and protein content, and plants have developed an array of chemical and 3 physical defenses that can limit nutrient accessibility. To compensate, herbivores have evolved a multiplicity of responses to cope with a mercurial and often nutri- tionally inadequate food supply. Response to Food Privation King and Murphy ( 1985) documented a number of compensatory mechanisms used by many animals to mitigate the effects of food privation resulting from either relative food shortage (food is plentiful but nutritionally inadequate) or general decline in resource abundance (food is unavailable or there is only a linrited amount). They include 1) accumulation and use of surplus nutrients, 2) economical use of available nutrients, and 3) a mixture of both (King and Murphy 1985). Surplus nutrients can be in the form of food caches that are often used by many animals during the winter. Hamilton (1941) reported finding nearly a peck (as 9 liters) of beechnuts in a Pemmyscus food cache, and Martin ( 1956) described underground chambers used by M. ochrogaster to store grasses and fruits that they feed on during the winter. 58 Hibernation and torpor are common methods used by many animals to con- serve endogenous energy stores and minimize dependence on exogenous food resources. Hibernation can reduce energy requirements to levels well below those required for normal activity. Herbivorous mammals such as marmots can reduce their basal metabolic rate by 92 percent during hibernation (Heldmaier 1989). Daily torpor can decrease daily energy requirements by up to 26 percent in Djungarian hamsters (Phodapus sungorus) (Heldmaier 1989) and up to 30 percent in white-footed nrice (Peromyscus leucopus) (Hill 1975). Note that the animals used in my research (prairie voles, M. ochrogaster) are not known to hibernate or go into torpor. Therefore, they are active throughout the day as well as year-round, and most likely must deal with food Shortages by methods other than metabolic arrest. Since they are herbivores, they may be faced with relative food shortages such as those caused by a seasonal decline in the nitrogen content of forage. It is possible, then, that herbivores such as prairie voles may have evolved adaptive mechanisms that allow them to subsist on a food supply that is highly variable in protein content and thus remain active throughout the year. Response to Dietary Fiber Plant fiber can limit digestibility and reduce a herbivore’s access to forage nutrients by dilution effects. In ruminants, the digestibility of cellulose and hemicel- lulose can range from 36 to 79 percent and lignin, cutin, and silica are essentially indigestible (Van Soest 1982). Some structural and functional alterations of the digestive system in herbivores are conspicuous adaptive traits that evolved to process bulky plant fiber. Most mammalian herbivores have sacculated digestive tracts that 59 facilitate the fermentation of large amounts of food by microbial action (McBee 1971). Large fermentation chambers permit an increase in retention time, which extends the time available for digestion. They also enable herbivores to host a large microbial population of bacteria, flagellates, and ciliates, which is essential for the digestion of cellulose and hemicellulose. These structural polysaccharides have B- glycosidic linkages that cannot be digested by most herbivores without the aid of microbial-mediated fermentation (Van Hoven and Boomker 1985). Other digestive tract adaptations include a highly papillated mucosal relief in the digestive compartments of ruminants for the absorption of large quantities of fermentive end- products. Forestomach fermenters such as ruminants often have comified epithelium covering much of their papillae in the rumen. Presumably, this increases the absorptive surface area (Van Hoven and Boomker 1985). Flattened molars used for grinding plant tissue and teeth that grow continuously to replace the loss due to wear from chewing abrasive plant material are common among mammalian herbivores. Insect herbivores have highly modified appendages for feeding on plants. Chewing mouthparts are used for fragmenting plant tissue, and many insects can penetrate the tough cell wall protecting the nutrient- rich cytoplasm by piercing it with a Sharp beak and sucking out the cell contents (Howe and Westley 1988). To augment nutrient uptake from low-quality forage, many herbivores increase food consumption and decrease throughput time, which increases the mass of food taken in for digestion. This is a common strategy employed by hindgut fermenters (Van Hoven and Boomker 1985). In foregut fermenters such as ruminants, the rate of l-'¢.' ;. I m 60 passage of food is limited by the rumination process. However, hindgut fermenters are less constrained because fermentation is not as extensive as in ruminants, and there are less sacculated compartments for food to pass through. Nonruminants such as the horse can process food twice as fast, for instance, as the ruminant cow (Bell 1971; Sibly 1981). According to Bell (1971): The importance of this difference emerges in a simple calculation: If the horse is two-thirds as efficient as the ruminant in extracting protein from a food but processes twice as much food in a given period of time, its rate of assimilation of protein per unit of time is four-thirds of the ruminant’s rate. E Therefore the horse (or the zebra) can support itself on a diet that is too low in protein to support a ruminant. A decline in digestive efficiency is usually associated with an increase in passage rate (Demment and Van Soest 1985). For instance, food in the horse is processed twice as fast as in the cow, but with 30 percent less digestive efficiency (Janis 1976). This has led some researchers to postulate that herbivores challenged by a low-quality, high—fiber diet, could benefit from increased gut size, thus enlarging the surface area available for assimilation of nutrients. An increase in gut surface area could increase digestive efficiency and counterbalance the reduction caused by low retention time. In a study by Gross, Wang, and Wunder (1985), prairie voles consuming high-fiber diets increased food intake and showed a significant increase in small intestine and caecal mass and length. Similar results for M. ochrogaster were obtained by Hammond and Wunder (1991), who postulate a homeostatic maintenance of digestive efficiency in small herbivores challenged by increased throughput. 61 Response to Allelochenricals Herbivores have developed many adaptations to counteract plant chemical defenses. These include behavioral and biochemical mechanisms. Behavioral mechanisms reduce exposure to allelochenrics, typically through selective feeding (e. g., Bergeron and Jodoin 1987). Learning plays an important role in foraging behavior for many herbivores, and ‘learned aversions’ to plant xenobiotics have been documented (Vickery 1984; Vaughan and Czaplewski 1985; Lindroth 1988). Interactions between forage constituents in herbivore diets can also be important. Selective consumption of plants that are high in saponins may reduce the binding of dietary protein by tannins due to chelation of tannin and saponin within the intestinal tract. In addition, the surfactant effect of saponins within the gut may prevent tannins from binding to the intestinal lumen and thus reduce tannin absorption and the resultant toxicity (Freeland et al. 1985). Feeding on a broad range of plants to nrininrize the toxic effects of any particular plant allelochemical may also be an effective behavioral strategy (McArthur, Hagerman, and Robbins 1991). Many enzyme detoxication systems that can overcome plant toxins are present in both insect and mammalian herbivores (Lindroth 1988; 1991). Detoxication can occur via gut microflora or by specific enzyme systems located in body tissues. Membrane-bound mixed-function oxidases are generalized enzyme systems capable of detoxifying many types of allelochenricals. They are present in several vertebrate organs such as the kidneys, intestines, and lungs, but are especially common in nricrosomes of the endoplasmic reticulum of liver cells (Lindroth 1988). They can also be found in fat bodies or the midgut of many insects (Howe and Westley 1988). 62 Their usual mode of action is to render foreign substances more hydrophilic via oxidation, reduction, hydrolysis, or conjugation reactions. This reduces the ability of a toxin to enter cell membranes, thus reducing toxicity and facilitating excretion (Lindroth 1988; 1991; McArthur et al. 1991). Inactivation of allelochemics can be accomplished via noncovalent complex formation with endogenous compounds. For instance, medicagenic acid from plant P. saponins is bound by bile cholesterol and excreted in the feces (Applebaum and Birk 1979). Salivary proteins excreted by ungulates and insects may minimize the fir .1" '. deleterious effects of tannins by binding with dietary tannin and forming inactive precipitates or soluble complexes (Bemays et a1. 1989; Robbins et al. 1991). Changes in gut pH can affect chemical reactivity of gut contents. The low pH found in mammalian stomachs can reduce tannin-protein complex formation (McAr- thur et al. 1991), and the highly alkaline conditions found in the midgut of some insects can cause tannins to dissociate from bound protein (Berenbaum 1980). Summary It is apparent that herbivore ecology is intimately linked to the quality of their food supply. The premise that herbivores cannot be limited by their food supply because “the world is green” seems, with hindsight, without widespread foundation. There is a wealth of evidence that suggests otherwise; food shortages—especially relative food shortages—are part of the fabric that molds the nutritional ecology of many herbivores. Also, herbivores and plants interact dynamically, exerting mutual 63 selection pressures that have shaped much of their ecology. Thus we see traits of both plants and animals that evolved because of reciprocal interactions. In the next section, I will discuss the relationship between the nutritional ecology of herbivores and metabolism. The determinants of mammalian basal metabolic rates are unknown. The effects of climate or animal size in relation to its surface area are often suggested to influence basal metabolism. Rarely are nutrient factors considered to play a role in determining basal metabolic rates (BMR). Since DIT in laboratory rodents is influenced by the balance of nutrients in the diet (Chapter I), it is reasonable to postulate that their relative abundance in the natural diets of herbivores can have some influence on the evolution of herbivore metabolic rates (either BMRS or gross metabolic rates) in the wild. To compensate for relative food shortages (low ambient dietary nitrogen), herbivores may have evolved physiological mechanisms such as DIT that elevate metabolic rates in response to low-protein food intake, which ostensibly could enable the distillation of specific nutrients from ‘unbalanced’ diets. METABOLIC RATE AND THE NUTRITIONAL ECOLOGY OF HERBIVORES The history of the scientific study of animal energetics extends back to the 17008, during the time of Priestley and lavoisier. Lavoisier found that oxygen was an elementary gas removed from ordinary air by both mice and a burning flame. This discovery could not easily be reconciled with the phlogiston theory currently favored by Priestly and others. The phlogiston theory held that phlogiston was added to air by a flame or a mouse and, in a confined space, eventually made the air unfit to E II." -. A 64 sustain either. This theory was eventually replaced by Lavoisier’s theory6 of combustion and metabolism, which stated that both a flame and a mouse use oxygen from air and produce heat in similar fashion. Each consumes oxygen by combining it with an organic substance, resulting in the production of C02, water, and heat. In 1780, Lavoisier and LaPlace concluded that most of the heat produced by an animal (in this instance, the guinea pig) is due to the in viva mixing of organic substances with oxygen. By the nineteenth century, the idea that combustion and metabolism were similar thermodynamic processes was firmly established (Kleiber 1975a). During the nineteenth century, physiologists discovered that the rate of heat production in endotherrns is highly correlated with body size. In 1839, Sarrus and Rarneaux proposed that mammalian heat production, as measured by oxygen con- sumption, was proportional to the free surface area and could be scaled to the ”A power of body mass. This became known as the “surface law” and was subsequently verified by many biologists. However, later research—especially in the twentieth century—showed that surface area and body mass alone could not account for the correlation of metabolism with body mass (Kleiber 1975a). Kleiber (1932) examined the BMRS of a number of animals, varying in size from rats to large ungulates, and described the relationship mathematically using an allometric function with an exponent of 0.74. The equation was subsequently revised as follows: “The English biologist Adair Crawford is considered the cofounder of this theory (McNab 1992). 65 M = 7ow°-75 where M = metabolic rate in kilocalories per day and W = body mass in kg (Kleiber 1947). General acceptance of the Kleiber equation as a standard for the study of animal energetics was one of the main factors that caused the decline in popularity of the surface law, since the surface law implied an exponent of 0.67 to describe the correlation between mass and metabolism in animals. The study of metabolism in the twentieth century branched off into many directions, permeating human physiological and nutrition research (e. g. , Kleiber 1945; Garrow 1978; Chapter I), as well as ecological research. Intensive studies of how temperature and climate affected thermoregulation and metabolism were undertaken (Scholander et al. 1950a; 1950b; 1950c). Animal energetics became a major area of study for environmental physiologists. Researchers used laboratory measurements of energy expenditures for various activities of wild animals (such as foraging, locomotion, and reproduction) to estimate field energy budgets (Pearson 1954; McNab 1963). This type of information was thought to typify the bioenergetics of animals under natural conditions and often formed the basis of initial calculations of trophic transfer and transformation of energy through populations and communities (e.g., Petrusewicz and Macfadyen 1970). Many scientists continued to pursue a physical explanation for the 0.75 exponent in the Kleiber equation (as of yet, without much success). Although the 0.75 exponent could not be satisfactorily explained, ecological explanations for variation above and below the Kleiber curve were pursued. Some focused on climatic correlates (Scholander et al. 1950a) while others examined phyletic relationships 66 (I-Iulbert and Dawson 1974; Crompton, Taylor, and Jagger 1978). Scholander’s group (1950a) concluded that “the basal metabolic rate of terrestrial mammals is fimdamentally determined by a size relation according to the formula Cal. /day = 70 kg. ", and phylogenetically nonadaptive to external temperature conditions. Equally nonadaptive is the body temperature, and the phylogenetic adaptation to cold there- fore rests entirely upon the plasticity of the factors which determine the heat loss, mainly the fitr insulation. ” Thus, there was little evidence to support the notion that climate controlled the basal rate of metabolism in animals (but see McNab and Morrison 1963, and Shkolnik and Schmidt-Nielsen 1976). Whether basal metabolic rates are set by taxonomic affiliation continues to be debated (Felsenstein 1985; Hays- sen and Lacy 1985) and will not be discussed here. Food Habits and Energetics in Mammals The search for the causal factors of variance in the Kleiber curve resulted in the finding that BMRS are correlated with mammalian food habits (McNab 1986). For instance, folivorous mammals have low BMRS, and the degree of depression is a function of the proportion of leaves in their diet (McNab 1978). McNab (1978) postulated three possible explanations: 1) leaves have high fiber and cellulose content and therefore are a poor source of energy, 2) arboreal mammals (a) reduce their intake of tree leaves because they are loaded with a variety of allelochenricals and (b) use glucose to detoxify allelochemicals which reduces net energy intake, and 3) arboreal folivores tend to be sedentary and have a low muscle mass, which could account for a reduction in basal rate. 67 McNab (1986) predicted that animals with a readily available, high-energy food source would have high BMRS. Those food sources with low digestibility, high allelochemic content, and a low energy content would permit only low BMRS. Grazing and meat-eating mammals were observed to have high BMRS (food may be readily available, high in energy, or low in toxins), while folivores, frugivores, and ant/termite-eating mammals had low basal rates (diet may have low availability, reduced energy content, and/or high toxin content). Those animals that consumed a mix of foods in the diet had intermediate rates (McNab 1986). McNab’s research was among the first comprehensive ecological studies to link metabolic heat production with food habits in mammalian energetics. However, our knowledge of this relationship is still limited. Recently, there has been some interest in the influence of allelochemicals on mammalian metabolic rates. Thomas, Samson, and Beregeron (1988) measured the metabolic rate of M. pennsylvanicus fed a diet of Purina rabbit chow containing 6 percent gallic acid (a phenol). Their results Showed an increase in metabolism by 13.6-22.6 percent above basal levels. Two factors were discussed that may have led to the increase in metabolism: 1) metabolic costs associated with detoxication and/or 2) the cost of tissue repair resulting from damage due to ingestion of a toxin. The metabolic cost of either of these processes is, however, unknown. Lindroth and Batzli (1983) have Shown that dietary tannin can increase uronic acid levels in the urine by 21-53 times over control levels, thus suggesting that tannins undergo detoxication via the glucuronic acid pathway. Detoxication by this method requires ATP and involves oxidation, reduction, or hydrolysis and conjugation with glucuronic acids or sulphates. Therefore, it is likely fi'. _ 68 that at least some of the elevation in metabolism resulting from ingestion of gallic acid is due to detoxication costs. Unlike McNab’S research (1986), which is based on correlative data, Thomas et al. (1988) measured a direct link between naturally occurring dietary constituents and metabolism. Because much of the focus in mammalian energetics has been on the measurement of BMRS, the specific effects of food intake on metabolism are rarely assessed, since basal metabolism is determined for a resting, fasting animal at tlrerrnoneutrality (Hill and Wyse 1989). Also, field energy budget measurements are usually calculated for the costs of specific activities, such as sleeping, walking, running, or flying. Thermogenic effects of food are usually not taken into account in these measurements. Energy budgets determined in the field by use of the doubly labeled water method (D20‘8) presumably include energy costs associated with food intake, but the specific energy costs associated with food intake are not determined by this method. Few ecologically oriented studies have examined the influence of food intake (amount of food consumed) and feeding habits (type of food consumed) on the gross metabolism of mammals. One such study, conducted by Thomas (1984) on paleotropical fruit bats (Pteropodidae), found that the amount of food ingested by pteropodids is determined by the protein content of the fruits in their diet. Typically, these bats ingest up to 2.5 times their body mass every day in fruit. By manipulating the energy and protein content in their diet, Thomas Showed that pteropodids’ food consumption is inversely related to the amount of dietary protein available per unit weight of food. 69 He also compared total food consumption of pteropodids to that of neotropical fruit bats (Phyllostomatidae) and found that, on a mass-specific basis, phyllostomatids ingest half as much energy, apparently being able to supplement their diet with insects to obtain the necessary protein. Thus, phyllostomatids can escape the nutrient constraints of a frugivorous diet that is naturally low in protein by supplementally feeding on a high-protein food source. In contrast, pteropodids are obligate frugivores and must increase food consumption to obtain an adequate amount of dietary protein. Although their mean daily energy intake is 194% higher than that recorded for a comparable phyllosto- matid bat (either captive or free-ranging), body mass does not increase nor is there a reduction in the uptake of carbohydrate from the gut. Thus, excess energy intake is disposed of metabolically, either by diet-induced thermogenesis (proposed by Thomas 1984) or by increased activity. Interestingly, both phyllostomatids and pteropodids have comparable BMRS (McNab 1982); they differ, however, in their daily energy budgets, presumably because of their feeding habits (Thomas 1984). Thomas developed a graphical model (Figure 2.1) that illustrates the hypotheti- cal relationship between energy and protein intake for neotropical frugivorous bats that facultatively consume different diets (insect, fruit, or mixed). This model is based on the ratio between energy and protein intake and predicts that when the dietary energy/protein ratio is above 199.7 kJ/g protein (61 .3/0.307), protein becomes a limiting nutrient and is the major determinant of food intake. Below the 199.7 value, bats regulate their intake based on energy needs. At 199.7, energy and protein needs are satisfied by one and the same food intake (mixed diet; Figure 2.1). 70 Asa. ass: a8. 3%.... egg? Essa Ea .888 a. as. 83... a 3.8.. 2. as 388. .o u 8... :8 .8... SEE . ”£588 .3 a E... .88.. £98.. :3. a. £3 a :25. a. a... o... 26.3 388 28. a. 4.8 so»... as. .2. 2.. .8... ea. 2.. .6 .808 388 .3 83538 a 23... .59 E. ass. 2a.... .8. 3o. a as ea 88: 538.. .8... as... 5 8 2E8. .2 a a". .38. Eats: a2. 3.3 we 8A a .8883? £22.. :3. 2.. 0.23 .3 as a .523? .388 :3. 2.. 52883 a. was: 8.. 8". a... .885 8 ea. 88... a 8 88. 39.3.53. 3.. .3 an. BEEBEE . 52833 88.5. .2953. 8.858.. a a. 23... a3 .. an... ESE .5 an. a use 532.. 862.. .88. .28.: .. .N 95...". o... 299... was 8. 2.39... a. Gamzm 2.... a.» >omwzm and: H55 Hanna. 5 gE-Dgfll . n . 3. .6203 T89 (er) mm Aeuana (Eur) MINI N13103:! 71 Food Habits and Energetic Efficiencies in Invertebrates The relationship between efficiency of food utilization and specific dietary nutrients is a common tOpic of research in the nutritional ecology of invertebrates. Researchers in this area tend to focus on measures of gross food consumption, rate of consumption, and the efficiency of conversion of ingested food to body substance, or ECI (Waldbauer 1968). ECI is calculated as follows: Body Growth or Production Ingested Food or Consumption ECI = x 100 Although ECI is not a measure of metabolic rate, energy loss via metabolic heat production significantly contributes to its value. All things being equal (e.g., digestive efficiency and activity level), as the ECI value decreases, the metabolic rate of an animal increases. Mattson (1980) plotted ECI values derived from a number of invertebrate nutritional studies against the nitrogen content of the consumer’s food supply and concluded that ECI and the nitrogen content of the food source are significantly related (Figure 2.2). According to Mattson, the data plotted in Figure 2.2 also imply that, to obtain an adequate supply of dietary nitrogen, food consumption must be higher for those animals subsisting on diets with low nitrogen availability. To illustrate the point, he mentions the energy consumption of xylem-sap-sucking insects, which consume about 100-1000 times their body weight per day of low-nitrogen sap. Also discussed was the hothouse nrillipede, a mandibulate arthropod with an ECI 72 value between 0.3 and 0.8 percent. This arthropod consumes five to six times its own body weight in food per day (Mattson 1980). Mattson examined four major studies of leaf-feeding insects using a covariance analysis to find the source of variation in the relationship described in Figure 2.2. ECI may be further broken down into two factors: approximate digestibility (AD) and efficiency of conversion of digested food to body substance (ECD). Therefore, 60F . 50- 40- . . . E . ' ESO- o . .o. . ' o of 20- 9 . 0.: .." . 10- ‘ o... oo . .o '1‘] l l l l l I 1 1 3 4 5 6 1 I NITROGEN CONTENT (s) or PLANT TISSUES Figure 2.2. Relationship between food nitrogen concentration and the efficiency of conversion of ingested food (ECI) by assorted invertebrate herbivores. (Graph from Mattson 1980.) another way to calculate ECI is as follows: ECI = AD x ECD where AD = (Consumption - Feces)/Consumption and ECD = Growth/(Consumption - Feces). This formula yields the same result as the formula for calculation of ECI previously 73 mentioned. Mattson’s analysis showed that most of the variation in ECI is due to ECD, not AD. This means that changes in ECIS associated with changes in plant nitrogen content are due primarily to post—digestive energy losses, i.e. , respiratory losses (Mattson 1980). In other words, the fraction of assimilated food that is oxidized increases as the protein concentration in the diet decreases. As in the fruit- eating bat study previously discussed, the source of increased metabolism could be either an intrinsic response analogous to DIT observed in mammals, or an increase in activity. Thermogenic Mechanisms and Dietary Protein In both mammalian and invertebrate research, there is no evidence to support a claim that activity and dietary protein levels are negatively correlated (although there is little, if any, research in this area). Thus, by appeal to parsimony, the increase in thermogenesis evoked by a decrease in dietary protein most likely occurs as a result of intrinsic physiological mechanisms. Also noteworthy is that it occurs in both vertebrates (in the forrrr of DIT as seen in laboratory mammals) and invertebrates (inferred from ECI changes previously described), and thus may be an example of convergent evolution, where different phyletic groups have adapted to a widespread shortage or imbalance of nitrogen or another nutrient in their natural food supply. However, the extent to which DIT occurs in wild mammals is unknown. Except for the study by Thomas (1984) of bats, the relationship between metabolic rates, feeding efficiencies, and dietary protein levels, and their influence on the nutritional ecology of wild mammals, is unexplored. 74 CONCLUSION The ecological framework above provides a foundation and impetus for DIT research on wild mammals. Currently, the most popular view in the nutrition literature is that DIT is a weight regulatory mechanism, not an adaptation to nutrient stress. Therefore, the focus of my research on DIT represents a perspective that has received little serious attention. My research places more emphasis on DIT as a physiological phenomenon that aids in protein uptake rather than weight regulation. While this perspective has not been widely stressed, there is evidence suggesting that it deserves more attention. Food generally is such a limiting factor in the wild that it is difficult to cast DIT evolutionarily in the role of a weight regulatory mechanism. Obesity is common in humans but less so in wild populations. Those animals that do store large amounts of fat (usually large animals) normally benefit through increased insulation and energy storage. A periodic scarcity of resources is common in nature, and wild animals often rely on stored fat depots for energy when exogenous resources are limited (King and Murphy 1985). Thus, it is counterintuitive from an ecological point of view that wild animals evolved a pathway to waste energy for weight regulation alone. However, if a low-quality, energy-rich food supply is readily avail- able, nutrient optimization may take precedent and provide the selective pressure for the evolution of DIT. CHAPTER III EXPERIMENTS AND ANALYSIS INTRODUCTION The animal chosen for this study is the prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster), a small rodent in the family Muridae, subfamily Microtinae. The prairie vole is a native of North America and is distributed from central Alberta to western West Virginia and northeastern New Mexico; it can also be found in southwestern Louisi- ana and southeastern Texas (Nowak 1991) (Figure 3.1). The prairie vole was chosen Figure 3.1. Distribution of Microtus ochrogaster. (Adapted from Hoffmann and Koeppl 1985.) 75 because it is primarily herbivorous and consumes large amounts of low-protein grasses and forbs to satisfy its nutrition- al needs (Batzli 1985). Also, prairie vole foodstuffs are not easily digested due to high fiber content, which adds to the challenge of obtaining adequate pro- tein uptake from a diet already low in protein content (Batzli and Cole 1979). Thus, prairie voles may be highly adapt- ed to feeding on low—quality foodstuffs because their natural diets are high in 76 fiber and low in nutrients. This makes them an excellent choice for studying metabol- ic adaptations to low-protein diets. Voles, in general, possess traits that appear to be adaptive to a herbivorous diet, especially a high—fiber, low-nutrient diet. High—crowned molars allow macera- tion of tough, fibrous vegetation, and continuously-growing molars compensate for wear caused by abrasive plant material such as silica. Voles also have an enlarged r4 caecum that functions as a fermentation chamber for digestion of fiber by symbiotic microorganisms (Batzli 1985). Voles exhibit a compensatory increase in intake when consuming foodstuffs L that are low in digestibility or nutrient content. This behavioral response is thought to allow voles to meet energy and nutrient requirements when feeding on low-quality food (Shenk, Elliot, and Thomas 1970; Batzli and Cole 1979; Batzli 1985). Compen- satory intake, along with the morphological characteristics mentioned above, may explain why voles do better on high-fiber, low-protein diets than most laboratory rodents (Shenk et al. 1970; Batzli 1985). However, even though voles are apparently well-adapted to herbivory, spatial and temporal variation in microtine densities in the wild may be linked to the quality of available forage (Batzli 1985). Cyclic declines in vole population densities occur every three to four years and may be related to low food quality encountered by voles during population peaks (Bergeron and J odoin 1987). If DIT occurs in small mammalian herbivores such as prairie voles, the ability to oxidize excess food intake to obtain an adequate supply of nitrogen would be ad- vantageous. This could allow them to increase their consumption of low-nitrogen 77 foodstuffs and lose the excess caloric intake as heat, thus increasing nitrogen intake. An intrinsic physiological response to low-protein intake such as DIT would be advantageous during times when the nutritional quality of resources is declining, such as during late summer, fall, and winter (Chapter H). In this view, energy is not a limiting resource for the prairie vole. Instead, the amount and quality of nitrogen in the diet places a limit on survivorship, reproductive success, and fitness. DIT would have considerable adaptive value by allowing an animal to consume an adequate level of nitrogen from foodstuffs low in nitrogen. This would be especially advantageous during gestation and periods of rapid growth, when nitrogen is essential for protein production. The aim of my research is to find out if DIT occurs in voles and, if so, how it might be adaptive under natural conditions. To accomplish this, voles at different stages of development were subjected to a variety of low- and high-protein diets, and their physiological and behavioral responses were recorded. Average daily metabolic rate (ADMR) and energy efficiency were measured using material balance and bomb calorimetry methodology; daily food intake was also recorded in one experiment. Biochemical change in brown fat was determined by GDP binding assay and used as an indicator of thermogenic activity of the tissue. Changes in the content of lipid and protein body tissue were studied to see if low metabolic efficiencies and/or increased food intake induced by a low-protein diet affected tissue deposition. Lipid content was measured by ether extraction, and protein levels were determined by Kj eldahl analysis. 78 Weanling voles (21 days old) were used in all experiments except for the first experiment. Since the growth rate of weanling voles is high, their protein require- ments are also high. Compared to fully grown voles, it is probable that weanling voles experience a greater amount of stress when dietary protein is limiting, since they require more protein to sustain growth. Therefore, if DIT is an adaptation that aids in protein uptake from low-protein rations, weanling voles are more likely than k ,_ adult voles to exhibit DIT in response to consuming a low-protein diet. This is the main reason most of the voles used in this research were weanlings. If DIT is ecologically important to small herbivores, then voles on low-protein diets, when compared to those receiving high-protein rations, should Show 1) low metabolic efficiencies, suggesting an ability to oxidize foodstuffs in excess of basic energy requirements (Kevonian et al. 1984), 2) heightened metabolic rates and brown adipose tissue (BAT) activity resulting from an increase in thermogenesis via the proton conductance pathway in BAT (Swick and Gribskov 1983), 3) a hyperphagic response, suggesting that voles increase food intake based on nitrogen requirements (Tulp et al. 1979; Donald et al. 1981), and 4) conservation of body protein and low deposition of body lipid, suggesting an intrinsic regulation of protein and lipid levels in the body during consumption of low-protein foodstuffs (Donald et al. 1981). Seven experiments were conducted to investigate the occurrence of DIT in prairie voles and the impact of a low-protein diet on their metabolism and behavior. The experiments consisted of three material balance studies, three experiments involving GDP binding assays, and an experiment that measured daily food consump- 79 tion and changes in body tissue (lipid and protein content). The following is a summary of those experiments and an analysis of their results. EXPERIMENT 1 Introduction Experiment 1 was exploratory. Since research on DIT had not been done using M. ochrogaster, many basic parameters were not known. Two primary factors that had to be determined were the total dietary ingredients that would sustain voles and be palatable, and the level of protein in the diet that might elicit a DIT response. Also, from the results of experiment 1, the influence of age and growth rate on metabolic rate in response to dietary protein could be determined; this information was critical for designing subsequent experiments. The main goal of experiment 1 was to see if the efficiency of energy retention and metabolic rate differ between low- and high-protein groups. Materials and Methods In experiment 1, 18 voles,’ ages 53-55 days, were divided into two groups and individually housed in metal, wire-bottomed metabolic cages, in a room maintained at 22°C with a 16-hour light/8-hour dark cycle. They were fed diets with casein contents of 7% and 17% for low- and high-protein groups, respectively. Diets were formulated for approximately equal metabolizable energy (MB) per gram of nutrients SThe prairie voles used in this research came from a Michigan State University colony. This colony was established from animals obtained from another colony at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL. 80 (excluding casein and glucose, which were variables in the diet). The estimated ME for casein is 16.7 kJ/g, while that for glucose is 15.2 kJ/g (Kevonian et al. 1984). Therefore, 100 g of 7% casein has approximately the same estimated ME as 99 g of the 17% casein diet. Diet ingredients were similar to those used by Kevonian et al. (Table 3.1). Table 3.1. Composition of diets, expt. 1. Mean ME intake Per vole Ingredients 7% casein 17% casein was different between groups (Table Casein’ 7-0 17-0 3.2) (P < 0.001, ANCOVA). This Glucose 74.3 63.3 . . difference IS probably due to a Cellulose2 4.0 4.0 Corn 011 10,0 10,0 difference in gross food consump- . . . , Vltamm m 1'0 1'0 tion. Because semisynthetic diets Choline bitartrate‘ 0.2 0.2 Mineral mix‘ 35 3.5 have a high digestibility (e.g., 100.0 g 99.0 g Hamilton 1939b), ME intake for animals fed these diets can often be lHigh protein from U.S. Biochemical Corp., Cleve- land, OH. 2Microcrystalline cellulose from Dyets, Inc., Bethlehem, PA. 3AlN-76 from U.S. Bio- . chemical Corp. ‘From Bio Serv, Frenchtown, NJ. f°°d consumption 03- Romsos, 5AlN-76 from Dyets, Inc. “Determined by bomb calorimetry. personal communication). Meta- used to estimate differences in gross bolic rates are affected by food consumption because when one animal consumes more food than another, the animal that consumes the most food will also incur the highest energy costs associated with food intake, such as the energy costs associated with mastication, digestion, transport of nutrients in the body, and biosynthesis of storage compounds. Therefore, ME intake was used as a covariate in the analysis of metabolic rate data derived from this experiment and experiment 3, which also had 81 unequal ME intakes. ANCOVA means for metabolic rates were adjusted so that each animal was compared at the same ME intake level, thus reducing the influence of ME intake on metabolic rates. This permitted a more accurate comparison of differences in metabolic rates that may be caused by differences in dietary protein levels. High-protein casein was used for the nitrogen source; variation in energy content of the diets resulting from different casein levels was Flow of may in the Body adjusted by changing the amount hm” , of glucose in the diets. All other 1 '7'. I N.” J ingredients—lipid, cellulose, g vitamins and minerals—were held l 4 I "Me-v I constant (Table 3.1). The ani- mals were fed ad libitum for 21 l 4 E: on days. Metabolic heat production was measured by material bal- _ Figure 3.2. The fate of ingested energy in an or- ance methods, where Ingested ganism. energy - waste energy :1; energy stored/ lost = thernric energy or energy of metabo- lism. However, ingested energy was not measured directly, since there was some spillage of food. Based on the chart of energy flow in the body (Figure 3.2), it follows that ME intake :1; energy stored/ lost = thermic energy or metabolic heat production. Therefore, ME intake was used to calculate energy of metabolism. ME intake was calculated by subtracting the energy losses in the feces, urine, and spilled food, from gross energy intake (total food removed from feeder). Feces, 82 urine, and spilled food (collectively termed as ‘waste’) were collected every three days. They were dried to a constant weight at 65°C, homogenized in a blender, and stored in the freezer in airtight containers until their energy content could be mea- sured. Using bomb calorimetry (Parr Instruments, Moline, IL), the energy content of urine, feces, and spilled food was determined for a 0.5-1.1 g aliquot‘5 of the homoge- nized waste mixture of each animal. In some instances, when urine production was large, urine was pipetted on cotton and bombed to determined its energy content (the energy content of cotton, determined separately by bomb calorimetry, was subtracted from the total energy recorded for the cotton/ urine combination). Body energy gain was calculated by subtracting final body energy from initial body energy. Initial body energy was estimated by linear regression of body energy on body weight of voles of similar age and weight as the experimental groups (see footnote to Table 3.2). This method was used in all the material balance experiments. DETERMINATION OF BODY ENERGY Voles were killed by C02 asphyxiation and their carcasses were homogenized in water, dried to a constant weight at 65°C, and stored in airtight containers. Body energy was determined by bomb calorimetry of a 0.2-0.9 g aliquot7 of the dried tissue homogenate of each animal. °Coefficients of variation (CV) were determined for waste sampling. Three waste aliquots per sample (mean = 0.67 g) from three different waste samples were used. CVS ranged from 0.011 to 0.019. 7Coefficients of variation were determined for tissue sampling. Three tissue aliquots per sample (mean = 0.48 g) from three different tissue samples were used. CVS ranged from 0.016 to 0.028. 83 Table 3.2. Metabolic rate, energy intake, and retention, expt. l. % casein in diet Low High 7% 17% Initial body wt, g vole'l 29.3 :1: 0.9 29.4 i 1.0 ME intake,l kJ vole'l 1394.1 :1: 86.6 1612.5 :t 83.5. Body wt gain, g vole" 1.7 i 1.0 3.0 i 1.0 Body energy gain,2 kJ vole" 86.2 :1; 33.2 78.6 i 42.1 Efficiency of energy retention,3 % 6.2 :1; 1.9 4.9 :1; 2.5 ADMR,"5 kJ d“vole'1 66.0 :1; 1.6 68.8 :1; 1.7 Wt specific ADMR} kJ d"g"vole’l 2.1 :1; 0.1 2.3 i 0.1 Values are means i SEM for 8-9 voles fed for 21 days. 1ME, metabolizable energy. 2Final body energy minus that estimated at the beginning of the experiment. Initial body energy was estimated by linear regression of body energy (y) on body weight (x) of 11 voles weighing 24-38 g (y = 16.0x - 151.2, 72 = 0.6). 3Body energy gain divided by ME intake times 100. ‘ADMR, average daily metabolic rate. 5ANCOVA adjusted least squares means, using ME as a covariate. ‘P < 0.001, ANCOVA. Results The data for experiment 1 are compiled in Table 3.2. All means were tested for statistical differences and only ME intake was found to be significantly different (P < 0.001, ANCOVA). Differences between means for initial body weight, body weight gain, and body energy gain were compared statistically using Student’s t test. ME intake was not the same for both groups (Table 3.2); therefore, it was used as a covariate in the calculation and statistical testing of ADMRS (kJ d"vole") and weight-specific ADMRS (kJ d“g“vole“), for reasons previously discussed. Meta- bolic rates were regressed on ME intake to test for homogeneity of slopes (Figure 3.3). The probability for significant interaction between ADMRS and the covariate 90 r r r T 80 70 ADMR (k1 / day) 60 50 900 1120 1340 1560 1780 2000 ME (kJ) Figure 3.3. Regression of the ADMR of low— and high- protein groups on the covariate ME intake to test for ho- mogeneity of slopes in an analysis of covariance (AN COVA) design. Slopes were not statistically differ- ent. P = 0.765. (ME intake) is 0.765 (P = 0.510 for weight-spe- cific ADMRS), so the as- sumption of homogeneity of slopes is reasonable. Covariate analysis of ADMRS showed no statis- tical difference (Table 3.2). Efficiency of ener- é gy retention (body energy gain divided by ME in- take times 100), a param- eter that is commonly used by nutritionists in energy balance studies, also did not differ. The Mann-Whitrrey U test was used for the statistical analysis of efficiencies (percentage data). Statistical testing was done with SYSTAT statistical software (Wilkinson 1989). The lack of difference in either ADMRS or efficiency of energy retention between treatment groups may have been due to the age of the animals used. Growth rates in the age span employed for voles may have been too low and variable to be affected by low-protein intake. These results were not unexpected since it was unlikely that older animals with slow growth rates would be under protein stress at the levels of protein provided in the experimental diets. 85 Since it was desired to experiment on voles at different stages of development, adult voles were used in experiment 1. One of the problems of using older animals in energy balance experiments where efficiency of energy retention is being measured, is that it is difficult to detect differences between treatment groups if growth is limited. If there was a difference in metabolic rates or efficiencies, it may have been too small to detect. Another possibile reason a DIT response was not observed is that older voles with low growth rates have less protein requirements for growth and therefore are less likely to exhibit a DIT response (assuming DIT is adaptive for increasing protein intake). Mean total growth was low in both groups for this experiment. It may be that the level of protein in the 7 % casein diet was too high to have a negative impact on growth. As a result, the protein level in the 7% casein diet may have been too high to stimulate DIT (again, assuming DIT is adaptive for increasing protein intake). EXPERIMENT 2 Introduction Experiment 2 was designed to test the effects of low- and high-protein diets on the metabolic rates and efficiencies of weanling voles. The weanling voles used in this experiment have a much higher growth rate than the adult voles used in experi- ment 1. Since protein is required for growth, it is possible that growing voles consuming low-protein diets may be protein limited. Therefore, they would be more likely than high-protein-fed voles to exhibit DIT (an increase in metabolic rate and decrease in energy efficiency per unit ME intake), if DIT augments protein uptake. 86 Materials and Methods In this experiment, 18 weanling voles, 21 days old, were used. Voles were divided into two groups and individually housed in metal, wire-bottomed metabolic cages, in a room maintained at 22°C with a 16-hour light/8-hour dark cycle. The fiber content in the diet was increased to 20 percent to simulate the high fiber content found in the natural diet of free-ranging voles (Fable 3.3). Voles exhibit improved performance (increased weight gain and more efficient nutrient utilization) when fiber is added to artificial diets, indicating an apparent need for bulk (Shenk et al. 1970). Based on the American Institute of Nutrition guidelines (1977) for laboratory [E animal diets, cornstarch was added to the diet to provide a complex carbohydrate source. The ratio of glucose to cornstarch was held constant for both groups. The amount of dietary casein for low- and high-protein groups was reduced to 5 % and 15 % respectively. Compared to experiment 1 diets, lower casein Table 3.3. Composmon of diets, expt. 2. . . I ed' ts 5% ° 15% ' levels made experiment 2 diets "gr lea casein case") Casein 5.0 15.0 more Similar 1n casein content to Glucose 42.2 35.2 diets used in previous DIT exper- ComSWCh 13-1 15-1 . . Cellulose 20.0 20.0 1ments (e. g., Kevonian et al. Corn oil 10.0 10.0 1984). Also, it was thought that Vitamin mix 1,0 1,0 a reduction in casein for the low- 010"” bitamate 0'2 0‘2 Mineral mix 3.5 3.5 protein group might be required 1000 g 100.0 g to induce a DIT response. Diets Gross energy, kJ/g 18.4 19.2 87 were formulated on a g ingredients /100 g mixture basis (Table 3.3). Voles were fed ad libitum for 21 days. ME intake, metabolic rates, and energy efficiencies were determined using the same material balance methodology described for experiment 1. Linear regression was used to estimate initial body energy (see footnote, Table 3.4). Preparation of body tissue homogenates and collection of waste material were done by the same methods described in experiment 1. Sampling procedure using single aliquots of tissue (0.2-0.6 g) and waste (0.8-1.1 g) homogenates was also the same. Bomb calorimetry was used to measure the energy content of aliquots of body tissue and waste. Differences between means for initial body weight, ME intake, body weight gain, body energy gain, ADMR, and weight-specific ADMR were compared statisti- cally using Student’s t test. The Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare efficien- cies. ME intake was not significantly different between groups (Table 3.4). There- fore, it was not used as a covariate when metabolic rates were compared statistically. Results Experiment 2 failed to show a significant difference in metabolic rates or energy efficiencies (Table 3.4). However, an interesting growth pattern was ob- served. The 5% casein group achieved growth similar to the 15 % casein group by the end of the experiment (Figure 3.4). Growth was initially slow for the low-protein group, but after 21 days, body weights were not statistically different. Change in body energy did not differ, suggesting that the relative composition of the body (fats, 88 Table 3.4. Metabolic rate, energy intake, and retention, expt. 2. % casein in diet Low High 5% 15% Initial body wt, g vole’1 19.3 :t 0.4 20.2 :1: 0.5 MB intake, kJ vole‘l 1644.3 :1: 93.7 1584.7 2%: 75.1 Body wt gain, g vole'1 7.9 j; 1.0 8.5 :t 1.1 Body energy gain,1 1:] vole’1 229.1 1; 23.8 212.2 :1: 30.1 L. Efficiency of energy retention, % 13.9 d: 1.3 13.4 i 1.6 ADMR, kJ d"vole‘l 67.5 :1: 4.0 65.4 :1; 3.0 Wt specific ADMR, kJ d“g"vole" 2.9 i 0.1 =..= 2.7 :1: 0.11 Values are means :1: SEM for 9 voles fed for 21 days. ‘Initial body energy was esti- mated by linear regression of body energy (y) on body weight (x) of voles weighing 17-21 g (y = 13.61: - 137.3, 1‘2 = 0.79). None of the values listed are significantly different. proteins, and water) was similar for both groups. The low-protein group could have increased mass by increasing lipid deposition, but since lipid has twice the energy/ g of tissue as protein, this probably would have been revealed by an increase in total body energy. Because of the initial slow growth rate in the 5% group, and the subsequent marked increase in growth rate occurring sometime after seven days into the experi- ment, it was hypothesized that there may have been an acute DIT response—possibly during the first 10 days of the experiment when growth was slow—which was obscured by an increased efficiency during the latter part of the experiment and therefore not detected. It is also possible that the level of protein fed to the low- protein group was still too high to elicit a measurable DIT response. Therefore, a 89 30 u T u 15 I- d 3 10 I- -1 A 15! and: 15 1 l 1' l U 3. (hub 11 21 33 41 A03 (an) Figure 3.4. Mean daily growth of weanling voles, expt. 2. Error bars are :1: SEM. third experiment was designed to test the influence of a lower level of dietary protein on metabolism and growth. EXPERIMENT 3 Introduction Since it was possible that a 5 % casein level was too high to cause DIT in voles (experiment 2), a 2.5% casein group was used in experiment 3 to see if a lower level of dietary protein could stimulate DIT. The 5% and 15% casein diet treatments used in experiment 2 were retained, making a total of three diet treatment groups used in experiment 3 (Table 3.5). Also, since an increase in the efficiency of energy retention after age 27 days (suggested by an increase in the growth rate after seven days, Figure 3.4) could have prevented detection of a DIT response occurring early in the experiment when energy retention may have been less efficient (suggested by the low growth rate during the first seven days, Figure 3.4), the duration of experiment 3 was limited to 10 days. Thus, experiment 3 differs from experiment 2 by being shorter in duration and having an additional diet group lower in protein than the 5 % casein treatment group of experiment 2. Experiment 3 was designed to test the effects of low-, medium-, 1‘... 90 Table 3.5. Composition of diets, expt. 3. Ingredients 2.5% casein 5% casein 15% casein Casein 2.5 5.0 15.0 Glucose 44.0 42.2 35.2 Cornstarch 18.8 18.1 15.1 Cellulose 20.0 20.0 20.0 Corn oil 10.0 10.0 10.0 Vitamin mix 1.0 1.0 1.0 L. Choline bitartrate 0.2 0.2 0.2 Mineral mix 3.5 3.5 3.5 100.0 100.0 100.0 Gross energy, kJ/g 18.4 18.5 19.1 it and high-protein diets on the metabolic rates and efficiencies of weanling voles over a feeding period of 10 days. Materials and Methods In experiment 3, 27 voles were divided into three groups and fed diets with casein contents set at 2.5%, 5%, and 15% (Table 3.5). Preparation of body tissue homogenates and collection of waste material were done by the same methods described in experiments 1 and 2. Sampling procedure using single aliquots of tissue (0.2—0.6 g) and waste (0.9-1.4 g) homogenates was also the same. Initial body energy was estimated by linear regression of body energy on body weight (see footnote to Table 3.6). Voles were fed ad libitum and housed under the same conditions as in previous experiments. 91 Statistical testing of means for initial body weight, body weight gain, and body energy gain were done by ANOVA. Post hoc comparisons of means were analyzed using Tukey’s HSD test. Means for efficiency of energy retention were compared using Kruskal-Wallis analysis of ranks (percentage data). Post hoc comparisons were done using the Mann-Whitney U test; differences were considered significant for P < 0.05/3 (Bonferroni correction). ME intake between groups was significantly different (P < 0.001, ANCOVA) and therefore was used as a covariate when metabolic rates were tested statistically by ANCOVA. The assumption of homogeneity of slopes was tested for metabolic rates analyzed by ANCOVA; slopes for ADMRS and weight- specific ADMRS regressed on ME intake were not significantly different (P = 0.10 for both). Post hoc comparisons of ANCOVA adjusted least squares means for ADMRS and weight-specific ADMRS were analyzed using Tukey’s HSD test. Results The metabolic and growth data are presented in Table 3.6. They show major differences in metabolism and weight change between the 2.5 % casein group and the other two groups, indicating a DIT response by the 2.5% casein group. High-protein- fed voles are more than four time as efficient at retaining energy as low-protein-fed voles. ADMRs (adjusted for ME intake by ANCOVA) for the low-protein group were almost 9 percent higher than the medium-protein group ADMRS, and over 16 percent higher than ADMRS recorded for the group fed the high-protein diet. The trend is similar and even more dramatic for weight-specific ADMRS, with voles fed low-protein diets having over a 40 percent higher weight-specific metabolic rate than 92 .28anme 8: u mz 5585 55.. u m ”5385 5585 u 2 ”5225 35 u .— 586 v m... .55 v 5.. .85 v .5. 955.60 a 3 m2 55.5 .2888 8556a 38— 8833 <>OUZ<_ and .I. «a £62 - 6.2 H .o m 3-5 wfinwmoa 86> .8 3 Emma? .33 :o S .388 32... .«o .8585on .825 .3 33:58 83 .388 .33 RES .926 3 58 8.5 83> m 58 2mm H 8.85 8.8 85:5 in: 555 US do H W 3 H 2“ 3 H «H 55375-5 2 .529. 058% .3 :5 .25 3 H 3m 3 H 93 I H 9% .8335. 2 229.. :55 .25 3 H 5.: 3 H ad ad H 3 5 .8558. 3.08 8 58505.5 .5: 5:5 .25 8.2 H S: 35 H 5.3 5.2 H QR .59, a .58 3.58 555 :52 5:5 525 3 H on no H 3 to H E- .52, m .58 E .585 .52 .5 in H mo; 3: H as: 35 H 3% 752, a .355 m2 m2 to H «.2 3. H we no H 92 .52, m .E 588 555 888855 e 2 g m R 2 585585 555 5582 33 55.. 5 59.8 5 .m .55 .8555. 55 .0555 5.58 .55 058502 .55 55:. 93 the high-protein-fed voles. These types of metabolic responses, (low energy efficien- cy and heightened metabolic rate) strongly suggest that voles increase metabolic heat 2‘ t I l I I 24- - 22- - WEIGHT (3) 3 k 1| - - A 15’ Casein O 55 Casein 1‘ I I I I I I I 2.5% Casein 21 23 25 21 29 31 A03 (den) Figure 3.5. Mean daily growth of weanling voles, expt. 3. Error bars are ;t SEM. production per unit of ME intake in response to low-protein feeding. Clearly, this is indicative of a low-protein DIT response analogous to those recorded for laboratory rats consuming low-protein diets (Chapter 1). Growth patterns, shown in Figure 3.5, were different in all three groups, re- vealing the importance of dietary protein in maintaining growth. Note that in the 5% casein group, the rate of growth increased sharply after eight days (29 days old), displaying a growth response similar to that observed in experiment 2. The 2.5% 94 casein group experienced weight loss during the first six days and then almost no growth thereafter. EXPERIMENT 4 Introduction Experiment 4 was undertaken to determine the physiological source of the thermogenic response shown by voles on a 2.5 % casein diet in experiment 3. This experiment was designed to assess the thermic activity of BAT in voles consuming 2.5% and 15% casein diets by measuring the GDP binding activity of the tissue. An increase in GDP binding is known to occur in rats consuming a low-protein diet (Swick and Gribskov 1983), and the assay is thought to be an indirect measure of BAT thermogenesis (Nicholls 1976). Materials and Methods Weanling voles were divided into two groups with 14 animals in each group, individually housed under the same conditions as those specified for experiments 1 through 3, and fed 2.5% and 15% casein diets for 10 days. Diet ingredients were the same as those listed in Table 3.5 for 2.5% and 15% casein groups. Voles were killed by cervical dislocation, and the interscapular BAT was quickly dissected and placed in a chilled buffer containing 0.25 M sucrose and 5 mM K-TES (pH 7.2). The tissue was then homogenized in glass homogenizing tubes with a Teflon pestle. Mitochon- dria were isolated by a procedure similar to that described by Cannon and Lindberg (1979), using successive centrifugations. Aliquots of mitochondrial suspensions were 95 assayed for protein content by a modified Lowry method using the Folin-Ciocalteu reagent and spectrophotometric measurement of absorbance (after Markwell et al. 1981). Bovine serum albumin was used as the standard. The association of 3H-GDP (radioactively labeled GDP) with BAT mitochon- dria was measured by the method described by Nicholls (1976), with some modifica- tions. Mitochondria (0.5 to 1.0 mg mitochondrial protein/m1) were added to an incubation medium at room temperature for seven minutes. Medium reagents were 100 mM sucrose, 1 mM EDTA, 20 mM K-TES, 2 uM rotenone, 100 pM potassium atractyloside, and 2.5 x 10" dpm/ml 3H-GDP (10.2 Ci/mmol, New England Nuclear, Boston, MA); 5.55 x 10’ dpm/ml MC-sucrose (673 mCi/mmol, New England Nuclear) was added to estimate the volume of medium trapped in the final mitochondrial pellet. The incubation medium was then divided in half; 200 uM of unlabeled GDP was added to one solution to estimate nonspecific binding of 3H-GDP. To the second solution, IOuM of unlabeled GDP was added to determine total binding of 3H-GDP. (Specific binding of 3H-GDP was calculated by subtracting the amount of 3H-GDP bound to nonspecific sites plus that trapped in the final pellet, from the total amount bound.) Tubes were quickly centrifuged at the end of the incubation period. The supernatant was aspirated and the mitochondrial pellet was dissolved using Beckman tissue solubilizer (BTS-450). The dissolved pellet was then suspended in a scintilla- tion cocktail8 and counted in a scintillation counter (TRI-CARB 4430 Liquid Scintilla- tion System, Packard Instrument Co. Inc., Downers Grove, IL). l"The scintillation cocktail was mixed using the following ingredients: 500.0 ml Triton X-100, 1000.0 ml toluene, 7.5 g PPO, and 150.0 mg DHPOPOP. 96 Results The specific binding for the low-protein group was 976 j; 62 pmol/mg mito- chondrial protein (:1; SEM); for the high-protein group it was 1149 j: 86 pmol/ mg. This was significantly different at P < 0.05, based on a randomized complete block ANOVA statistical design, with blocking on assay days. While these differences are statistically significant, there may not be much biological importance in the differenc- es. Both groups had very high levels of GDP binding—much higher than previously recorded for laboratory rats exhibiting DIT—indicating a high level of BAT activity for both groups. These values fall well within the range of GDP binding measure- ments taken by Klaus, Heldmaier, and Ricquier (1988) for winter-acclimated wild species. For example, seasonal peak mean values recorded for Clethrionomys were 1337 j: 67 pmol/mg; for Apodemus they were 1164 i 101 pmol/mg (values are :1; SEM) (Klaus et al. 1988). Presumably, the high values recorded by Klaus et al. were due to the effects of cold on nonshivering thermogenesis (NST). While high GDP binding values recorded for the low-protein group in experiment 4 could have occurred because of DIT, the cause of the similarly high values recorded for the high- protein group cannot easily be explained. EXPERIMENT 5 Introduction Experiment 4 showed that voles on a 2.5% casein diet had high specific GDP binding levels, which were indicative of heightened BAT activity. However, this experiment was inconclusive because the 15% casein group also had high specific 97 GDP binding. One possible explanation for these surprising results is that both groups may have been thermoregulating by NST. If NST was occurring, it could have affected the activity of BAT and, hence, the binding of purine nucleotides. The ambient temperature during experiment 4 was approximately 22°C. According to Webster (1983), at 24°C there is a 36 percent increase in metabolic heat production by mice housed in a metal calorimeter when compared to mice housed in normal cages. The voles in experiment 4 were housed in wire-bottomed metal cages, which would make them highly susceptible to increased heat loss by conduction and convection. Interestingly, when I first began using wire-bottomed metabolic cages, I measured the metabolic rates of C3H laboratory mice housed in these cages to test material balance methodology. During the test, three mice became hypothermic and had to be revived in an incubator. It may be that the voles in experiment 4 were using NST to maintain body temperature, and the results of the assay were clouded by elevated GDP binding levels due to NST. Because DIT and NST both cause an increase in GDP binding, the effects of DIT on GDP binding could be masked if NST is appreciably active. Therefore, the goal of experiment 5 was to see if voles held at the same temperature but under different caging conditions would show a difference in GDP binding to brown fat mitochondria. The hypothesis was that voles housed singly in wire-bottomed metal cages should be under greater cold stress because they are more susceptible to conductive and convective heat loss than animals housed as a group in plastic cages supplied with bedding, since group-housed voles could huddle and be more thermally insulated. As a result of the greater cold stress, voles housed in metal 98 cages may exhibit NST and therefore have higher GDP binding levels than voles housed in plastic cages. Materials and Methods To test the hypothesis that N ST may have caused a substantial increase in GDP binding in voles placed in metabolic cages at T, = 22°C, housing status was used as a variable and GDP binding was compared. Voles housed as a group in plastic cages supplied with bedding were compared with those housed individually in metabolic cages. There were 12 animals in each treatment (single or group). Both treatment groups received rabbit chow ad libitum and were maintained at an ambient temperature of 22°C for 10 days. GDP binding was determined by the same proce- dure used in experiment 4. Data were tested using a randomized complete block ANOVA, with blocking on assay days. Results The mean GDP binding value recorded for group—housed voles was 961 j; 107 pmol/ mg mitochondrial protein (1: SEM). Voles housed individually showed a specific binding of 1712 j: 102 pmol/mg (i SEM). These values were significantly different at P < 0.001. The results of this experiment suggest that housing status can influence the specific binding of GDP to BAT mitochondria, presumably as a result of thermoregu- latory costs. More important, high levels of GDP binding due to NST in voles housed individually in metabolic cages at 22°C could obscure differences induced by diet. 99 EXPERIMENT 6 Introduction Homeotherms at thermoneutrality can regulate their body temperature by nonevaporative physical processes without increasing their metabolic rate. The range of ambient temperatures this occurs in is called the thermoneutral zone (TN Z), which has boundaries set by upper and lower critical temperatures (Bligh and Johnson 1973). Once the ambient temperature ('1‘.) drops below the lower critical temperature of the TNZ, a homeotherm must elevate its rate of internal heat production to preserve a constant body temperature. This is usually accomplished by an increase in shivering or nonshivering thermogenesis. At temperatures just below the TNZ, NST is usually the dominant form of heat production in wild mammals, whereas shivering is activat- ed at much lower temperatures (Jansky 1973). NST induced by cold acclimation in small mammals has been linked to BAT thermogenesis (Foster and Frydman 1978), and BAT mitochondria of cold-acclimated animals exhibit an increase in purine nucleotide binding (Desautels, Zaror-Behrens, and Himms-I-Iagen 1978). Since GDP binding by BAT mitochondria is used as an indicator in testing for both DIT and NST, the potential for confusion exists, especial- ly when GDP binding is used as an assay in energy balance studies where animals are kept at temperatures below thermoneutrality. The results of experiments 4 and 5 suggest that the cool ambient temperature in the laboratory (22°C), coupled with poor insulative properties of metabolic cages, induced NST and caused the high specific binding of GDP to BAT mitochondrial protein observed at both dietary protein levels 100 in experiment 4. Experiment 6 was conducted to examine the influence of dietary protein on specific binding of BAT mitochondria when voles are held at thermoneutrality. Materials and Methods Weanling voles were individually housed in plastic cages supplied with bedding and placed in an environmental chamber. T, was set at 28°C, a temperature close to the lower critical temperature for prairie voles’ (W under, Dobkin, and Gettinger 1977). They were maintained at a 16-hour light/8-hour dark cycle and fed diets with casein contents of 2.5% and 15%, with eight animals in each group. Diet ingredients were the same as those listed in Table 3.5 for 2.5% and 15% casein groups. After 10 days, the animals were sacrificed and specific GDP binding by brown fat mitochondria was assayed as in experiments 4 and 5. Results Means for specific binding of GDP were 524 j; 72 and 712 i 86 pmol/mg for low- and high-protein groups, respectively. These values were not significantly different (P = 0.12), based on Student’s t test of independent sample means. A com- plete block ANOVA was not used for this analysis as in previous experiments, 9An exact value for the lower critical temperature was not determined for the voles used in this experiment. The lower critical temperature for an animal can vary depending on an animal’s age, size, pelage, and prior acclimation or acclimatization history. In the study by Wunder, Dobkin, and Gettinger (1977), it was assumed that 27 .5°C was the lower critical temperature for wild-caught prairie voles. However, not all the animals tested in that study were at thermoneutrality at 275°C. Since an exact value for the lower critical temperature was not known for the animals used in experiment 6, 28°C was arbitrarily chosen, which is slightly higher than the one chosen by Wunder et al. (1977). 101 because data recorded on separate assay days were not significantly different (P = 0.10). These data suggest that the content of protein in the diet does not influence BAT activity via activation of the proton conductance pathway when voles are tested at thermoneutrality. It is not known if metabolic rates and efficiencies differed between the two groups, since an energy balance study was not performed at this temperature. Also, there is little information in the literature concerning the influence of dietary protein on the metabolism of small mammals at thermoneutrality, although some experiments involving cafeteria feeding of rats at 29°C show that DIT and GDP binding are suppressed (Barr and McCracken 1982; Rothwell and Stock 1986b). The GDP binding data for voles provide no evidence that the low metabolic efficiencies of the 2.5 % casein group recorded in experiment 3 resulted from activa- tion of the proton conductance pathway in vole BAT mitochondria. Furthermore, it is likely that NST contributed significantly to the high levels of GDP binding observed in experiments 4 and 5, since GDP binding is much lower for voles at thermo- neutrality. However, comparisons of GDP binding data between experiments may be inappropriate, due to the sensitivity of the assay to local conditions. EXPERIMENT 7 Introduction Food consumption, gross nitrogen efficiency, and body tissue changes were measured in this experiment. Food consumption was measured to determine the relationship between food protein content and feeding behavior. Tulp et al. (1979) 102 and Donald et al. (1981) noted an increase in weight-specific food consumption for rats fed low-protein diets when compared to those on high-protein diets, and Shenk et al. ( 1970) observed that meadow voles increase intake when dietary protein levels are low. Nitrogen efficiency data will give a general idea of the relationship between dietary protein content and the efficiency of its utilization in the body. The data will be limited though, because net nitrogen efficiency was not measured. Data on body composition will show how tissue constituents are affected by the protein content of the diet. Donald et al. (1981) found that adult rats fed a high- protein diet had significantly more body fat than those on a low-protein diet, even though gross food consumption was similar for both groups. This type of response may indicate that animals on low-protein diets conserve dietary protein and fuel energetic costs primarily by the use of non-protein calories. Materials and Methods As in experiment 3, 27 voles were divided equally into three dietary groups (2.5%, 5%, and 15% casein, made up as in Table 3.5). Voles were individually housed in metal, wire-bottomed metabolic cages, in a room maintained at 22°C with a 16-hour light/8-hour dark cycle. Food consumption was measured daily for three weeks. The feeders used in this experiment were designed to minimize food spillage and eliminate contamination of food by feces and urine. Fwders were constructed by gluing a length of 2 inch diameter SCH 40 PVC pipe to a plexiglass base. The length 103 of the pipe depended on the height from the floor to the roof of the cage. The pipe was out long enough so that the final height of the fwder was sufficiently high enough to prevent an animal from entering the feeder through the gap between the roof of the cage and the top of the feeder. Four feeder holes, 3% inch in diameter, were drilled in the side of the pipe, so that the bottom of the holes was 1% inch above the base. A section of ‘A inch SCH 40 PVC pipe, cut to the same length as the 2—inch pipe, was glued to the plexiglass base inside the feeder so that it was mounted parallel to - the 2-inch pipe. The tube was placed close enough to the feeder holes to prevent an animal from crawling into . the feeder, but far enough away to allow the animal to stick its head into the V feeder to eat (approximately 14 inch L J from the feeder holes). The overall Figure 3.6. General design of feeders . used in expt. 7. See text for details of desrgn of the feeders forced voles to construction. feed while standing on their hind legs. The food level in the feeder was kept 56 inch below the feeder holes, so that voles could not dig the food out with their front paws, but they could extend their heads into the fwder to eat (see Figure 3.6). Voles were sacrificed by C02 asphyxiation and gut contents were removed. Carcasses were softened by autoclave for 30 min and then homogenized and dried as described in experiment 1. Carcass lipid levels were assayed using the Soxtec System HT6 (Tecator AB, Hogana, Sweden). Three to five aliquots (061.4 g) of tissue 104 homogenate per animal were used for lipid determination. Protein content was mea- sured by Kjeldahl analysis of aliquots (0.2-0.3 g) of fat-free tissue homogenates. Three aliquots of fat-free tissue homogenate per animal were used for protein determination. Due to the light, powdery nature of the food, it was impossible to avoid some spillage. Even with carefully designed feeders, two animals were able to dig out significant amounts of food that added to the waste. To reduce the variance associat- ed with spillage, it was assumed that a large ‘consumption’ of food was actually the result of an excessive waste. Therefore, based on total food consumption, all the food consumption data collected for two outliers (one from the low-protein group and one from the high-protein group) were eliminated from the feeding data set (see footnote, Table 3.7). The two outliers were the same two animals that were able to dig their food out of the feeders. Initial weights of body tissue and water components were estimated by linear regression lines derived from the analysis of nine voles matched by weight and age to the experimental groups at the start of the experiment. Individual body constituents (protein, lipid, water, and other10 (y variables» were regressed on body weight (x), generating the following regression equations: y = 0.13x - 0.08 for protein, y = 0.11): - 0.73 for lipid, y = 0.71x + 0.76 for water, and y = 0.05): + 0.05 for other. Calculations derived from these equations are presented in Table 3.9 (Initial protein, lipid, water, and other weights) and were used to compute tissue and water changes. 10Other is assumed to be primarily skeletal in origin (Evans 1973). 105 .32 :8 9855 59505 65999 5&8 5.3 do .8523“. 33 bESSEK Legs .638 \e «eecgh DMD 5.85:5 9553 58 829 1855.5 «.3 God u 3 newneaaoo v8.5 :58 co v9.3 59:53 Eat 355650 803 £053 25,—. .256 V m... .86 V m: .86 v m. .53 mm: H.355. wean 0:8 So? 288 he 885888 8e 36% .<>Oz< .3 38:98 203 58 :< 38> 9w 58 2mm H 28:. 0.8 82‘; mz o4 H 35 45 H 05 as H 5.5: _.9o>_.w 2 .935 85 be a .55 08 H 9:8 «.9. H @585 4.2: H 5.82 _.9o> 5 .935 89 be 8 m2 3 H «.5. 2 H 3.. E H 99. .99.-» a .935 88 be 2 :52 5:5 :25 99. H 92 S: H o3 «.8 H E. .99, a .935 88 be 2 ”859955 52 em .92 885:me :5: 5582 33 H5. 5 598 9 .5 .55 50:55:88 89 0589-559» Ba 309 .2“ 9%... 106 E H pa 0 DAILY ENERGY common on vol-"1 8 8 IO A 15‘ Canal: I 5! Caaaia l 2.5! Caaala 7° - - - - 15! Caaaia — — - 5! Caaala ‘0 15’ Mn 0 5 10 15 20 25 DAYS Figure 3.7. Mean daily food consumption, expt. 7. a 12 I a 11 z 10 9 y . a 15G Caada O 5! Caul- 1 l 2.5! Caaala ’ - - - - 15. Cal-II - - - 5! Caada g ‘ 2.5: each Figure 3.8. Weight-specific mean daily food consumption, expt. 7. 107 Results Table 3.8. Post hoc polynomial contrasts, expt. 7. F001) CONSUMPTION GROUP CONTRASTS PROBABILITY Total food consumption Willi—119.11% LH P = 0.001 was calculated for the first 10 LM P < 0.01 days and for the full length of MH NS the experiment (21 days) (Table W 3.7). Results show that 10- and LH P < 0.001 21-day total food consumption LM P < 0.001 was greatest for the 15% casein MH r NS group, when compared to the ‘P < 0.001 (repeated measures ANOVA). 2.5% group. Weight-specific food consumption was the same for all three groups during both periods. An examination of mean daily food consumption reveals a steady increase in food intake by the low-protein group and a decline in consumption over time for the high-protein group (regression lines, Figure 3.7). By the end of the experiment, mean daily food consumption was highest in the low—protein group. The increase in food intake by voles fed the low-protein diet is more evident on a weight-specific basis (regression lines, Figure 3.8). Repeated measures ANOVA revealed a signifi- cant difference in food consumption over time for both whole-animal and weight- specific daily food consumption (P < 0.001). Polynomial contrasts indicate different feeding patterns occurred between the low- and medium- and low- and high-protein groups for both the whole animal data and per-unit-weight data (Table 3.8). 108 Table 3.9. Body composition of voles fed 2.5%, 5%, and 15 % casein diets, expt. 7. Initial wet wt Final wet wt Change wet wt Initial protein wt Initial lipid wt Initial water wt Initial other wt Final protein wt Final lipid wt Final water wt Final other wt Change in protein Change in lipid Change in water Change in other is] E 1"! Protein Lipid Water Other DI! Weight; (g) Initial dry wt Final dry wt Change in dry wt 0 Protein Lipid Other Fr Ini' 2.5% 5% 15% 20.7 3}; 0.5 20.8 ;t 0.4 20.7 i 0.4 21.2 :1: 0.6 26.8 :1: 0.9 30.6 :1: 1.2 0.6 :1: 0.4 6.0 :1: 0.9 9.9 :t 1.2 2.6 :1; 0.1 2.6 :t 0.1 2.6 :l: 0.1 1.5 i 0.0 1.5 i 0.0 1.5 :l: 0.0 15.5 i 0.4 15.6 i 0.3 15.6 :1: 0.3 1.0 :1: 0.0 1.0 :1: 0.0 1.0 i 0.0 2.8 :t 0.2 4.2 :1; 0.2 4.8 :t 0.2 2.8 :1: 0.4 4.3 :1: 0.4 5.2 d: 0.7 14.4 i 0.4 16.5 :1: 0.4 18.6 i: 0.6 1.2 :1: 0.1 1.9 :1; 0.2 2.1 :1: 0.2 0.2 :1: 0.2 1.5 :t 0.2 2.1 :1: 0.2 1.3 j; 0.3 2.8 :l: 0.4 3.7 :1: 0.7 -1.1 i 0.4 0.8 :t 0.1 3.0 :1: 0.6 0.2 :1: 0.0 0.9 :1: 0.2 1.1 :t 0.2 8.0 i 6.4 58.6 i 7.8 80.7 :t 6.5 82.4 :1; 20.0 184.3 1 22.7 246.3 :I: 47.9 -7.0 :t 2.8 5.6 :1: 3.0 19.9 :I: 3.9 20.1 i 5.6 85.8 :1: 18.3 105.2 :1: 15.1 5.2 :t 0.1 5.2 :l: 0.1 5.2 i 0.1 6.8 :t 0.4 10.4 :1; 0.6 12.0 :1; 0.9 1.7 i 0.3 5.2 i 0.6 6.9 :t 0.9 4.1 :1; 3.3 30.0 :1: 4.0 41.4 j; 3.4 67.2 :t 20.0 53.0 :1: 5.2 49.0 :t 5.8 69.7 i 22.2 55.0 i 8.1 50.0 i: 5.6 17.6 :1: 15.3 30.8 3: 3.6 34.4 :1; 4.3 67.2 i 19.9 53.0 i 5.2 49.0 i 5.8 15.2 :1: 5.0 16.2 :1: 2.3 16.6 :1; 2.2 ——= 109 BODY COMposITION Significant differences were found in weight gain between all groups (Change wet wt, Tables 3.9 and 3.10); weanling voles fed 15% casein diets gained the most (Figure 3.9). Data for the underlying tissue and water changes are compiled in Table 3.9; ANOVA and post hoc comparisons are listed in Table 3.10. All percentage data Table 3.10. Statistical analysis of selected body composition data listed in table 3.9, COMPOSITION TEST Post hoc comparisons Wts (g) Change wet wt ANOVA P < 0.001 I H, P = 0.001; LM, P = 0.001; MH, P < 0.05 Final protein wt ANOVA P < 0.001 I H, P < 0.001; LM, P < 0.001 Final lipid wt ANOVA P = 0.010 I H, P < 0.01 Final water wt NOVA P < 0.001 I , P < 0.001; LM, P < 0.05 Final other wt W P = 0.001 I , P = 0.001; LM, P < 0.01 Change in protein ANOVA P = 0.001 I H, P < 0.001; LM, P < 0.001 Change in lipid ANOVA P < 0.01 I , P < 0.01 Change in water ANOVA P < 0.001 I , P < 0.001; LM, P < 0.05; MH, P = 0.01 Change in other W P = 0.001 I H, P = 0.001; LM, P < 0.01 5 Change From Initial Protein P < 0.001 I , P < 0.001; LM, P = 0.001 Lipid P < 0.05 I M, P < 0.01 Water P < 0.001 I H, P = 0.001; LM, P < 0.01; MH, P = 0.012 Other P = 0.001 I , P < 0.001; LM, P < 0.01 [2g Weights (g) Final dry wt ANOVA P < 0.001 I H, P < 0.001; LM, P < 0.01 Change in dry wt [KW P < 0.001 I H, P < 0.001; LM. P = 0.001 fl Change From Initial Protein W P < 0.001 I H, P < 0.001 Lipid NS Other W NS Co sition of T' Q . Protein W NS Lipid Other KW = Kruskal-Wallis analysis of ranks. 110 were analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis analysis of ranks (KW), as were data that violated homogeneity of variance (Bartlett test). Post hoc comparisons for data analyzed by KW were accomplished using the Mann-Whitney test; differences were considered significant for P < 0.05/3 (Bonferroni correction). Tukey’s HSD test was used for ANOVA post hoc comparisons. The data in Table 3.9 indicate that body weight and body protein content Table 3.11. % Composition of final wet and dry weights, expt. 7. % COMOPSITION GROUP :1 Low Medium High Significant Final Wet Wt 2.5 95 5% 1596 Differences Protein 13.4 1; 0.7 15.6 i 0.3 15.7 :t 0.6 LM‘ Lipid 11.8 :1: 1.5 15.9 i 1.1 16.4 :t 1.8 NS Water 67.9 i 1.4 61.5 :t 1.2 61.1 :1: 1.6 LM" LH‘ Other 6.9 :1: 1.2 7.0 i 0.5 6.9 :1: 0.5 NS PM Dg Wt Protein 42.3 i 2.9 41.0 n 1.7 41.0 :1; 2.6 NS Lipid 39.5 i 3.8 40.9 i 2.2 41.3 i 3.5 Ns Other 18.1 i 0.9 18.1 i 1.0 17.7 :1; 1.2 NS Percentage data analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis analysis of ranks. Post hoc comparisons were done using the Mann-Whitney test; differences were considered significant for P < 0.05/3 (Bonferroni correction). 'P < 0.05, 'P < 0.01. increase with increasing dietary protein. Lipid, water, and other show similar trends. These results are comparable to those found in studies of rats (Donald et al. 1981; Edozien and Switzer 1978). The proportion of protein, lipid, and other dry tissue gain did not differ (% Composition of Dry Tissue Gain, Table 3.9). This resulted in no change in the relative proportions of dry tissue constituents for the whole animal (Table 3.11). Percent composition of final wet weight showed that protein was proportionately 111 “TTTIIITIIIII 30- n 3 (D E 20.. ‘7 .- AlSiCaaain IS‘Caaain 10 IIILIIIIIIII IZSICIIOIII 212325212931333531554142 AGEldaya) Figure 3.9. Growth of weanling voles, expt. 7. Error bars are :1; SEM. reduced in the 2.5 % casein group. Even though this group lost water (% Change From Initial, Table 3.9), the final carcass water content was proportionately greater than the water content in either the medium- or high-protein group (Table 3.11). Consequently, the percent protein in the final wet weight of the low-protein group was diminished by a relative increase in water content. Note that the relative proportions of protein, lipid, and other in the final dry weight were equal for all three groups (Table 3.11). This is interesting because total food consumption and growth rates were markedly different. 112 .506 V m: .56 v m. .386 88: a 588 .99 a: £93 55 <65. .3 5:55 555% .85 955 z 565 .3 825“. Saw Z boom. 5885.898 85 we magmas 85 8 .888qu :2: 358 Z 33 85m... .Eouootoo 58.8.5508 Qmod V m 58 5:85:me 3.8288 883 8088.553 :85 8523-582 85 mean 8:8 883 3on .8 885958 .63 3.6m 5555 Co 595528 5583-355 .3 “88:98 San—N .mmd \ 9 5888 u z .53 u m .8. - an; n e w 8-: 555563 85> e .8 8 5563 5.8 .5 8 59:8 5985 5.8 CO .85mb8 58:: .3 “88:58 33 539:” .33 35:: .5335 238.9 .3 “858.88.. 283 8253 Z =< .52 .25 2 H 02! m5 H Won 8 H 3. 5. 5.88858 2 580 :5 :25 58 H 3% 4.8 H 2.8 98 H can .96., we 3.56» 2 58m :52 :5 :5 QB H mama 4.: H 45; 35 H 39. .92, me .6555 2 5555 m2 ed H 29. 45 H 3.5. I: H 93. .99, we _.z .68 E55 8288th R. m _ a» m R m .N 585255 e55 5582 33 one 5 598 9 H 598 .8558 comp—5: 3.80 .S .m 838. 113 NITROGEN BALANCE Gross nitrogen efficiency was highest for the 5 % casein group (Table 3.12). Gross nitrogen efficiencies were statistically the same for low- and high-protein groups, even though dietary nitrogen intake was six times greater in the high-protein group. However, net efficiencies are not known, since fecal and urinary nitrogen contents were not measured. DISCUSSION Introduction The focus of the proceeding experiments has been to examine the effects of different dietary protein levels on energy balance, brown adipose tissue activity, food consumption, body tissue change, and nitrogen balance in herbivorous voles. Low energy efficiencies were recorded in experiment 3 for voles consuming low-protein diets (Table 3.6). Voles fed low—protein diets had efficiencies that were over 75 percent lower than voles fed high protein diets. The low efficiencies observed in experiment 3 for low-protein-fed voles are characteristic of a physiological phenome- non known as diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT), which has been observed in labora- tory rats consuming low-protein diets (Chapter I). The effector organ for DIT in laboratory rats is thought to be brown adipose tissue (BAT). Specific-binding of GDP to BAT mitochondria is a parameter used to estimate BAT activity. Contrary to what has been observed for laboratory rats, specific binding of GDP to BAT mitochondria does not increase in voles fed low- protein diets when compared to high-protein-fed voles (experiments 4 and 6). 114 Food consumption data (experiment 7) indicate that voles increased food intake when fed low-protein diets. Gross food consumption was greater for high-protein-fed voles (Table 3.7), but over a 21—day feeding period, the rate of food consumption increased for voles on low-protein diets and declined in the high-protein group (Figures 3.7 and 3.8). Body composition data (Table 3.9) from experiment 7 indicate that body weight, body protein content, lipid, water, and ‘other’ tissue were higher for voles consuming high-protein diets when compared to animals fed low-protein diets. However, the relative proportions of protein, lipid, and other in the final tissue dry weight showed no difference based on the level of protein in the diet (Table 3.11). Gross nitrogen efficiency did not differ between low- and high-protein groups but it was significantly higher for the medium—protein group when compared to the other two groups (Table 3.12). Dietary nitrogen intake was six times greater in the high-protein group compared to the low protein group. Net nitrogen efficiency for the three treatment groups was not determined. Thermogenesis Experiments 1 through 3 were designed to test the influence of dietary protein on the utilization of energy in voles. Similar experiments have been performed on rats (e. g., Stirling and Stock 1968), and revealed an increase in metabolic heat production and reduction in efficiency of energy retention. Likewise, data presented here show that the protein content of the diet is a major determinant of energy efficiency in voles. 115 A 7% casein diet was not low enough to elicit a significant thermogenic response in voles in experiment 1, probably because the animals used in this experi— ment were too old to be stressed by protein deficiency at the level offered. Once voles reach 30 g, they are considered adults (Baker 1983), and growth thereafter is highly variable and relatively slow when compared to the first 30 days post-weaning (personal observation). The voles used in experiment 1 (53-55 days old, with a mean weight of 29 g), having almost reached adult weight, were at a stage where growth is slow and unpredictable. Note that in the 17% casein group, the mean in growth was only 3 g over a period of 21 days. Thermogenic responses to low-protein rations have been recorded in adult rats (Donald et al. 1981; Swick and Swick 1986). In the study by Donald et al., the 10- week-old rats used in the experiment were considered ‘fully grown, ’ but mean weight gain over a period of 9 weeks ranged from 100 g for rats fed a 5 percent protein diet to 200 g for rats fed a 25 percent protein diet. Donald et al. measured final fat-free body mass but the initial fat-free body mass was not estimated. Final fat-free body mass, though, was greater than the initial weights of the animals. Presumably, some of the growth was due to an increase in body protein. It is likely, therefore, that protein intake was important for the continued growth that occurred in these rats. The researchers’ main focus, though, was the influence of dietary protein on adiposity in older, ‘nongrowing’ rats. Consequently, little attention was given to changes in body protein. The question remains as to whether ingestion of a low-protein diet would induce a thermogenic response in animals that no longer require dietary protein to sustain growth of protein tissue. 116 Since protein turnover can be substantial in fully grown animals (Swick and Benevenga 1977), presumably there is a minimum level of dietary protein required by these animals for maintenance. If DIT stimulated by low-protein intake is an adaptive process for the distillation of dietary protein, then fully grown animals could exhibit DIT when protein intake falls below maintenance. In DIT experiments using laboratory rats, a 5 % casein or lactalbumin diet is usually low enough in protein content to induce a thermogenic response (e. g. , Donald et al. 1981, Swick and Swick 1986). DIT in rats has also been detected using 8% casein diets (Tulp et al. 1979; Rothwell et al. 1983a). However, in experiment 2, a 5 % casein diet was not low enough in protein content to produce a significant increase in thermogenesis in voles. There are at least two possible explanations, which are not mutually exclusive. There may have been an early DIT response and low energy efficiency that was obscured by a subsequent increase in efficiency of energy retention occurring after seven days into the experiment. It is also possible that voles, known to have a high BMR when compared allometrically to other animals, require less dietary protein to stimulate DIT. Since voles are herbivores, they may have evolved elevated BMRs to aid in dissipation of excess energy intake resulting from consumption of large amounts of grasses that are typically much higher in energy content than in nutrients such as protein. The growth pattern in experiment 2 (Figure 3.4) suggested that some type of compensatory response occurred that enabled the 5 % casein group to catch up to the 15 % group after a comparatively slow growth rate at the start. Energy efficiencies for the whole growth period were nearly identical, so there was no prima facie 117 evidence suggesting that DIT was involved. Since the experiment 1 ran for 21 days, and the growth rate for the 5% q casein group increased after 7 days into the experiment, it is con- 0 ‘ 2.5! 5.0. 15.0! ceivable that a DIT pa 0 I I met (Dom! ENERGY GAIN/In) x 100 response occurred ini- Figure 3.10. Efficiency of energy retention, expt. 3. . . Error bars are :1: SEM. tially dunng the first 7 days, and then was masked by subsequent growth and an increase in efficiency. Figure 3.10 shows the energy efficiencies for experiment 3. The 2.5 % casein group differed statistically from the other two groups, but the 5% and 15% casein groups showed no significant difference (P = 0.047 for post hoc comparison using the Bonferroni correction of P = 0.05/ 3). Due to the large variance inherent in material balance studies, if a DIT response occurred in the 5% group, it may have been too small to be detected statistically. However, the data in Figure 3.10 suggest a trend, with energy efficiencies decreasing as dietary protein is reduced. Also of note, the mean efficiency for the 5 % group in experiment 3 for a 10 day period was lower than that recorded in experiment 2 for 21 days (12.0 versus 13.9 percent, respectively). It is doubtful that this is demonstrably different, but the mean change 1 18 is in the direction predicted for a DIT response occurring during the first 10 days of feeding. As mentioned earlier, another explanation relates to a vole’s BMR. Voles may require a lower dietary protein level than laboratory rats to stimulate DIT, if elevated BMRS known to occur in voles aid in dissipation of excess energy intake. Voles have been shown to have a 20 to 40 percent greater BMR than expected from allometry (Wunder 1985). McNab (1980) suggested that an abundant high-energy food supply for herbivores permitted the evolution of higher BMRS. A high BMR is positively correlated with a variety of fitness traits, including rm, the maximal intrinsic constant of population growth, suggesting that a high BMR is somehow adaptive (McNab 1980). According to McNab (1980), “. . . it behooves all mammals to have as high a rate of metabolism as can be sustained by the quantity and quality of their food resources in space and time, because this adjustment will permit them to maximize their reproductive efforts. ” It is also possible that herbivorous feeding not only permits a high metabolic rate, it necessitates it. If we assume that dietary nitrogen—not energy—is the most common limiting resource for a herbivore (Chapter 11), then higher BMRs could facilitate processing of foodstuffs and extraction of essential nutrients to meet nutri- tional requirements. A high metabolic rate that aids in the assimilation of nutrients could be an essential trait required to evolve herbivory. If we compare herbivorous voles to omnivorous rats, it may be that voles are more metabolically adapted to process foodstuffs that are nutritionally unbalanced by having a higher than normal basal metabolic rate. Herbivorous voles may not have to increase metabolism at the 119 7% (experiment 1) or 5 % (experiment 2 and 3) casein level because their metabolism is already 20—40 percent higher than comparable omnivorous homeotherms that may be less limited in nature by the nitrogen content of their food supply. Thus, the low level of nitrogen in the diet required to increase thermogenesis in voles may be the result of an already intrinsically high BMR. The data from experiment 3 (Figure 3.10) clearly indicate a DIT response by the 2.5% casein group, and suggest that voles require lower concentrations of dietary protein to elicit a significant increase in thermogenesis than omnivorous rodents such as rats. F urther- more, a comparison of energy expenditures in rats versus voles on low-protein diets shows that weight—specific ADMRS are several times higher in voles. Rothwell and Stock (1987c) reported an energy expenditure of 0.9 k] d"g‘1 for rats maintained at 24 j; 1°C and fed low-protein dies for 15 days. This contrasts sharply with the 2.7 to 3.8 k] d“g’l ADMRS recorded in experiments 1 through 3 for voles on either high- or low—protein diets, indicating a comparatively high weight-specific metabolic rate for voles. Although the rats used by Rothwell and Stock (1987c) were considerably larger, it is unlikely that the difference in weight-specific metabolic rates was due to size difference alone. The relationship between weight-specific BMRs (M) and body weight (W) for birds and mammals can be expressed by the following allometric equation: M = 70W“ where M is in k] kg"d'l and W is in kg (Kleiber 1975b). The mean weight for voles on a low-protein diet in experiment 3 was 18.3 g and the mean weight for laboratory 120 rats fed a low-protein diet in the study by Rothwell and Stock (1987c) was 151.5 g. When these mean weights are used to calculate M for the low-protein-fed voles used in experiment 3 and the laboratory rats on low-protein diets used by Rothwell and Stock (1987c), the results indicate that the smaller voles should have a weight-specific BMR that is 70 percent greater than that of the larger laboratory rats. Experimental data (experiment 3 and Rothwell and Stock 1987c) indicate that the 18.3 g voles had an ADMR that was 333 percent higher than the 151.5 g laboratory rats (3.8 k] d“g‘1 versus 0.9 k] d"g“, respectively), thus indicating that the empirically derived ADMRS did not scale allometrically according to the above equation. These calculations also suggest that body weight alone is not responsible for the differences in weight-specific ADMRS between voles and rats. According to Kleiber (1975b), the weight-specific metabolic rate reflects the turnover rate of chemical energy in the body. Because of their high metabolic turnover rates, voles consuming low-protein diets are capable of rapidly processing large amounts of ingested energy. If nitrogen is a limiting resource in the natural diets of voles, a high metabolic turnover rate evoked by foodstuffs deficient in nitrogen could facilitate nitrogen uptake by permitting high food intake and a concom- itant distillation of protein, while oxidizing unneeded nonprotein energy. The results from the energy balance experiments performed here support this hypothesis. Brown Adipose Tissue One of the most exciting and novel aspects about the DIT phenomenon is the idea that it is connected with BAT metabolism. Not only is there a substantial rise in 121 heat production associated with cafeteria feeding or low-protein intake, the mechanism of heat production ostensibly occurs by an unusual process circumventing ATP production in specialized brown fat tissue. In 1979, when Rothwell and Stock pub- lished their seminal paper on DIT in Nature, linking the phenomenon to BAT metabolism, a new approach to the study of energy balance was engendered. Previ- ously, BAT had been assigned the role of the principal effector of NST. Since BAT thermogenesis had already been the subject of considerable research as a thermoregu- latory process, many of the techniques used to elucidate BAT activity in response to cold could now be used in energy balance studies (Chapter I). One of the most common techniques used to estimate BAT activity is the GDP binding assay. Binding of purine nucleotides to thermogenin, the uncoupling protein in the mitochondrial membranes of brown fat, is thought to be part of the regulatory process that determines proton conductance across BAT inner mitochondrial mem- branes and, therefore, thermogenic activity. This assay has been applied to DIT studies using both cafeteria and low-protein feeding regimens, and the results tend to show that GDP binding and DIT are positively correlated (Chapter I). An increase in GDP binding, however, was not observed in voles feeding on low-protein diets. GDP binding at 22°C was greater in experiment 4 for the high- protein group compared to the low protein group; at thermoneutrality (28°C), no significant difference was recorded between dietary groups (experiment 6) (Figure 3.11). As previously mentioned, though, a difference in specific binding was observed for voles experiencing different thermal conditions created by disparate housing conditions (experiment 5). 122 15. u v 1 u 11:. + . I l no - i . g 1. - + .. g 5“ - 4 fl 1 l l l m It! LI IV m4. 4.. s “I «our Figure 3.11. GDP binding, expts. 4 and 6. L = low protein, 2.5% casein; H = high protein, 15% casein; C = cold (22°C); W = warm (28°C). Error bars are i SEM. Since voles in experiment 4 had very high GDP binding levels, and those in experiment 5 were significantly different—ostensibly due to different levels of NST resulting from microclimate differences—it was concluded that testing voles at thermoneutrality would eliminate variation in GDP binding caused by NST. The experiment at thermoneutrality, however, did not uncover differences in GDP binding relative to dietary protein differences. The most likely conclusion is that BAT proton conductance was probably not responsible for the low energy efficiency observed in experiment 3 for voles on the low-protein diet. Rothwell and Stock (1986b) have argued that DIT is inhibited at high environ- mental temperatures, probably to prevent hyperthermia at thermoneutrality. They 123 found that rats fed cafeteria diets and kept at 29°C showed no evidence of DIT. Barr and McCracken (1982) had similar findings. Thus, it may be that voles at thermoneutrality failed to show a significant difference in GDP binding due to diet because the thermogenic response was suppressed. Perhaps a better experiment would be to test voles at temperatures only slightly below thermoneutrality, thus minimizing NST, yet reducing the chances of hyperthermia. More difficult to explain are the GDP binding results observed in experiment 4, where both low- and high-protein groups had high GDP binding levels, and GDP binding for the high-protein group was significantly higher. Swick and Swick (1986) noted similar results for rats fed 5% and 15% lactalbumin diets for 3% months. Specific GDP binding to brown fat mitochondrial protein (pmol/mg protein) did not differ statistically, but total GDP binding (nmol/IBAT pad“) did. According to Swick and Swick (1986), total GDP binding is a more reliable parameter for measuring BAT thermogenesis, because total interscapular BAT is accounted for. However, total GDP binding was not measured in voles; therefore, the difference in total binding for voles on low- and high-protein diets is not known. Total GDP binding for BAT in voles consuming variable protein diets should be measured before any conclusions can be made about the contribution of BAT metabolism to DIT in voles. Complicating matters are recent studies suggesting that heat production by BAT is not increased by cafeteria feeding. Foster and Ma (1989) and Ma and Foster (1989) made direct measurements of BAT oxygen consumption using nonocclusive "IBAT pad refers to the interscapular brown adipose tissue pad. Total GDP binding is determined for the total amount of BAT dissected from the animal. 124 cannulae to sample the oxygen content of venous effluent from BAT. Radioactive microspheres were used to measure tissue blood flow. Cannulas were also placed in the right common iliac artery to sample arterial oxygen content. BAT oxygen consumption was calculated using the Fick principle. Their results show that cafete- ria-fed rats exhibiting a measurable DIT response did not exhibit a significant elevation in BAT metabolism. Heat production in BAT contributed only 2 to 3 percent of the total thermic effect of cafeteria feeding. Foster and Ma also found that partial hepatectomy (’6) had a major effect on whole-body metabolic rate of cafeteria- fed rats, with a reduction in V0, equal to or greater than the increase in V0, caused by DIT under prehepatectomy conditions in cafeteria-fed rats (Foster and Ma 1989). Since it is possible that the liver may be an effector organ for DIT, the degree to which BAT is involved in heat production via DIT may not be fully known. In a review article on BAT thermogenesis, Himms-Hagen (1990) stated, “In no case has it been possible to quantitate the contribution made by BAT to the altered state of energy balance.” According to Himms-Hagen, some rats with atrophied BAT do not become obese, while others become obese, even though their BAT content increased and is thermogenically active. Perhaps the main reason the GDP binding results of experiment 4 seem contrary is that our expectations are based primarily on studies of laboratory rats. It is possible that, in some species, specific GDP binding may not be a suitable assay for BAT thermogenic activity stimulated by low-protein DIT. Clearly, more interspecific comparisons are needed. Both specific and total GDP binding should be measured in more species exhibiting DIT, so that the two assays can be compared for 125 effectiveness in predicting BAT thermogenic activity among different animal groups. Until more information is known about DIT-related BAT metabolism in species other than laboratory rats and mice, it is premature to generalize about the contribution of BAT to low-protein DIT based on the GDP binding data presented here. Food Consumption One of the main hypotheses in this research is that voles should show a hyperphagic response while fwding on low-protein rations. A hyperphagic response would allow voles to increase protein intake by increasing bulk intake. A concomi- tant reduction in energy efficiency would permit voles to eliminate unneeded calories while extracting maximal dietary protein. Clearly, a hyperphagic response is illustrated in Figures 3.7 and 3.8. They show that the rate of intake increases steadily for voles on a low—protein diet and declines over time for the high—protein group. Also, by the end of 21 days of feeding, voles on the low-protein diet are consuming as much or more food per day than their high-protein counterparts, even though there is a mean difference in weights of 9.4 g, which constitutes a 44 percent greater body weight for the high-protein group. However, looking at gross food consumption (Figures 3. 12-3. 15 and Table 3.7), the 15% casein group ingested the most energy during the first 10 days and for the 21-day total; weight-specific food consumption did not differ during either period. The only time voles on a low-protein diet had significantly greater energy to dispose of by DIT than high-protein-fed voles was during the last few days of the 21-day V\\\\\\\\\\\\\\m F P b h 9.13%ng sumption for the first 10 days of expt. 7. Figure 3.13. Mean total food con- 126 , w\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\s_ $8“ nnn 0.13%:ng Figure 3.12. Mean total food con- sumption for 21 days of expt. 7. total food consumption for the first 10 Figure 3.15. Mean weight-specific days of expt. 7. total food consumption for 21 days of Figure 3.14. Mean weight-specific expt. 7. 127 feeding period, when their daily consumption was highest (Figures 3.7 and 3.8). However, the low energy efficiency recorded in experiment 3 for low-protein-fed voles was for a lO—day period that corresponds to the first 10 days of experiment 7, thus making it difficult to attribute the source of DIT to oxidation of gross energy intake. Since DIT occurred in low-protein-fed voles during a time when their food intake was lower than voles fed high-protein diets, it is more likely that the primary cause of thermogenesis induced by low-protein diets in voles is the ratio of protein to energy in the diet and not gross energy intake alone. Rothwell and Stock (1987c) came to a similar conclusion after a study of rats fed low-protein diets showed increased thermogenesis even though ME intake‘2 did not differ. Other data suggesting that low-protein DIT occurs in the acute time frame can be seen in growth patterns (Figures 3.5 and 3.9). At the outset, the growth rate is considerably greater for high-protein groups. While this may be due to low food intake by the low-protein group, it could also be, in part, the result of low efficiency of energy retention by the low-protein group when compared to the high-protein group. Energy balance studies shorter in duration or detailed measurements of daily metabolic rates (oxygen consumption) would be nwded to resolve this question. Glick et al. (1981) suggested that a DIT response recorded in an energy balance study may be caused by daily heat production via meal-induced thermogenesis (i.e. , specific dynamic effect or heat increment of feeding). A typical energy balance l2Rothwell and Stock (1987c) corrected ME intake for body size differences (kl/(kg0'75d)). Actual ME intake (intake before allometric adjustment for body size) was different between treatments, with low-protein groups showing the smallest ME intake. 128 study can last for one or more weeks, and the final energy balance determination represents energy efficiency integrated over the duration of the experiment. Accord- ing to Glick (1981), the observed DIT response measured over time represents the cumulative effects of heat production resulting from by specific dynamic effect. Glick (1987) cites as evidence that most of the increases in BAT parameters resulting from DIT (e. g. , hypertrophy, thermogenesis, GDP binding, and norepinephrine turnover) have also been found to occur after the intake of a single meal. Thus, Glick and colleagues see DIT as specific dynamic effect writ large. From this perspective, the acute thermic response exhibited by voles on a low-protein diet in experiment 3 is more understandable. Because of the magnitude of the difference (energy efficiency was more than four times greater for the 15 % casein group), it is more likely that the low efficiency recorded over a period of 10 days is a cumulative response, rather than occurring in the just last few days of the experiment. The ‘excess’ energy wasted by voles may be derived from a nutritionally unbalanced series of meals, where daily energy consumption above maintenance is disposed of by oxidation. In this view, DIT is an ongoing process that functions in the acute time frame, and it is elicited by nutritionally imbalanced fwding bouts or meals. If this is the case, it would mean that small herbivores such as voles can rapidly adjust their metabolism to local conditions of food quality. This could confer an adaptive advantage to herbivores living in environments where high quality food is patcth distributed or food nutrients are temporally variable. The results of the energy balance study of experiment 3 combined with food consumption data indicate that DIT observed in voles occurs quickly in response to a 129 diet unbalanced in protein. However, this does not negate the possibility that voles can exhibit DIT as a chronic response to overlngestion of energy. Clearly, daily food consumption continued to rise after 10 days, but note that weight gain was only minimal (Figure 3.9). This suggests that low energy efficiencies continued after 10 days and may have been even lower that those recorded initially. Therefore, it is possible that voles can make both short- and long-term adjustments to their metabolic rates to meet nutrient constraints imposed by changing levels of dietary nitrogen. Body Composition The body composition data (Tables 3.9 and 3.11) indicate the importance of dietary protein in protein tissue gain. The 15 % casein group gained 10% times more body protein. The high-protein group also gained almost 3 times more lipid and 5% times more ‘other,’ which is probably a result of their high total food intake (Figure 3.12). Low-protein-fed voles gained only a small amount of weight (0.6 g), but their dry tissue gain was 1.7 g. This was due primarily to water loss of 1.1 g. The low- protein group gained protein, lipid, and other in the same proportions as the other two treatment groups, and the final relative proportions of dry tissue were almost identical (Table 3.11). It could be that growing voles homeostatically maintain the relative proportions of solid tissue in their body when faced with a food supply that is low in nutrients. Overall growth could be aided by distillation of dietary protein via DIT, which would permit an increase in the growth of protein tissue, and, consequently, other tissue. If an innate proportional constraint to tissue growth exists, it might also explain why 130 balanced diets result in the greatest growth increases. Excess energy that cannot be used to maintain proportional growth in body tissues could be eliminated via DIT. However, this is purely hypothetical and more research would be needed to test these suppositions. Nitrogen Balance Mean nitrogen efficiency for the low-protein group (7.6 percent) in experi- h. ment 7 exceeded the overall energy efficiency recorded for the low-protein group in experiment 3 (4.0 percent), although it is doubtful that this difference is significant. It does suggest, though, that voles are able to differentially increase nitrogen efficien- .1; cy over total energy efficiency. Mean nitrogen efficiency for the 5% casein group in experiment 7 was 30.3 percent, while their energy efficiency recorded in experiment 3 was 12.0 percent. Two voles in the low-protein group lost body nitrogen; one vole lost almost as much as the maximum gain recorded for the 2.5% group. Thus, considerable variation in nitrogen efficiency occurred in the low-protein group, suggesting that the nitrogen content of a 2.5% casein diet was near the lower limit for maintenance of positive nitrogen balance. When dietary nitrogen was doubled at the low end of the spectrum (2.5% to 5%), gross nitrogen efficiency quadrupled. Thus, small increases in nitrogen in the diet are significant when the total protein content of foodstuffs is minimal. Given that food consumption by the low-protein group was increasing daily, it may be that eventually, an increase in intake could have supplied enough nitrogen to 131 cause a significant increase in growth rate. Some evidence supporting this notion can be observed in the growth curve for experiment 7 (Figure 3.9). After initially losing weight, growth in the low-protein group began to rise. This may have been due to the steady increase in food consumption, which means greater nitrogen intake. If higher nitrogen efficiencies occur with only moderate increases in nitrogen intake, then a low energy efficiency coupled with an increase in food consumption would translate into an effective physiological and behavioral system for optimizing nutrient L intake from a high-energy, nutrient-poor diet. CHAPTER IV CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS It has been suggested that diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT) is an adaptive response that regulates energy balance and weight in mammals (Rothwell and Stock 1979). Proponents of this idea have labeled the phenomenon as ‘adaptive’ DIT (Rothwell and Stock 1986a). However, because our understanding of this physiologi- cal response is still limited, it is probably premature to use a nonneutral adaptive label for DIT. By calling it adaptive DIT, we assume we know why it is adaptive. However, several adaptive functions have been proposed for DIT, with no clear demarcation between them (Himms-Hagen 1986). DIT is evoked by excess energy consumption and by moderate ingestion of a low-protein diet (Rothwell and Stock 1979; Rothwell et al. 1987c). It is not clear what differences, if any, exist between these responses, nor have the possibilities for different adaptive functions been fully explored. DIT occurring in response to cafeteria or low-protein feeding is usually treated as the same phenomenon, even though it can be elicited by different feeding regimens. To complicate matters, there are some who dispute the existence of DIT, and believe that the thermic response observed in laboratory rodents to cafeteria feeding is an experimental artifact (Barr and McCracken 1982). Furthermore, the term ‘adap- tive’ has several meanings and can be interpreted differently by researchers with 132 133 different backgrounds. Rothwell and Stock (1981a) have implied that DIT is an evolutionary adaptation, but the most common use of the term adaptation in the nutri- tion literature is to denote a phenotypic physiological adaptation (Waterlow 1985) and DIT could be interpreted as such. Therefore, to avoid confusion, minimize contro- versy, and foster a sense of objectivity, a specific adaptive label for DIT should be avoided. I Since DIT is stimulated by low-protein intake, it has been postulated that it i could be adaptive for increasing nutrient intake by distillation of essential nitrogen from low-protein foodstuffs by oxidation of excess, nonprotein energy intake. Several % researchers have made this suggestion, but an ecologically-based scenario for the genetic selection of DI’T as a Darwinian adaptation to nutrient stress has not been fully articulated. Most of the current discussion in the nutrition literature is focused on the impact of DIT on energy balance and obesity in laboratory rodents (e.g., Himms-Hagen 1989). The role DIT plays in nutrient optimization has received little serious attention. The extent to which DIT is involved in augmenting nutrient intake may best be examined by focusing on the physiology and nutritional ecology of herbivores. Herbivores consume foodstuffs that are typically low in nitrogen. Food quality may be so poor (low nitrogen content) that herbivores are unable to consume enough nitrogen to satisfy maintenance needs or nourish young (Sinclair 1975). Temporal changes in food quality and availability due to climatic changes such as rainfall, can drastically reduce the amount of high-quality, high-nitrogen food available to herbivores. Chemical defenses of plants can force herbivores either to 134 feed on less nutritious parts of plants or consume plants of lesser quality. Feeding can also be deterred by structural defenses such as spines or trichomes. Digestible protein can be diluted by high dietary fiber, and allelochemicals such as tannin can reduce nitrogen availability by binding with dietary protein. Thus, the amount of high-nitrogen foodstuffs available to herbivores in the wild can be limited and have a negative impact on herbivore survival, suggesting that selection pressure could lead to the evolution of specific traits that allow herbivores to cope with low ambient levels of dietary nitrogen (McNeil and Southwood 1978; White 1978; Mattson 1980). Laboratory rats have been the primary animals used in research on low-protein 1' ' DIT. Since these animals are omnivores and are not as likely as herbivores to be limited by food resources low in nitrogen in the wild, it is not clear how ecologically important an increase in thermogenesis in response to low-protein intake would be for them. A study of wild frugivorous bats, though, suggests that they can overingest energy to obtain sufficient protein, and dispose of the excess energy by oxidation (Thomas 1984). This is significant because the bats studied by Thomas ( 1984) are obligate frugivores that must rely on nutrient-poor fruits (typically 5 3.3 percent protein by dry weight) to fulfill their nutritional demands. Thomas’ study showed clearly that food intake and energy efficiencies of frugivorous bats were dependent on the protein content of the diet. The model devel- oped by Thomas (Figure 2.1) predicts that frugivores are significantly more likely to be limited by dietary nitrogen than omnivores. By comparing an obligate with a facultative frugivore, Thomas was able to show the ecological implications of a thermogenic response caused by low-protein intake. His research also emphasized the 135 importance of using frugivorous or herbivorous wild animals in this type of study. Linking the nutritional ecology of an animal directly to an observed physiological response facilitates the interpretation of the results and lends credibility to the supposed function of that response. In this case, Thomas’ data strongly suggest that, for those species constrained by the nutrient quality of the food they consume, DIT could play a significant role in the ability to achieve an adequate nutrient intake. Nutritional studies of invertebrates provide further evidence that nitrogen can be a limiting nutrient in the wild and that animals have developed specific adaptations to deal with this problem. Mattson’s analysis of invertebrate herbivores showed that i the efficiency with which a consumer can convert ingested food into body tissue is positively correlated with the percent nitrogen in the diet (Mattson 1980) (Figure 2.2). He also showed that the low energy efficiencies observed in insects feeding on plants low in nitrogen are not due to a decrease in throughput time, but are caused by post- digestive processes, thus suggesting metabolic changes similar to DIT observed in mammals. That similar responses occur in both homeotherms and poikilotherms indicate that DIT stimulated by low-nitrogen foodstuffs could be a ubiquitous response to a widespread problem of nitrogen shortages in the wild and may be evolutionarily convergent. Low energy efficiencies recorded for herbivorous voles in the experiments de- scribed in Chapter III provide further evidence that a DIT response to low-quality food is an adaptation that aids in nutrient uptake. Complete material balance studies of voles on variable protein diets show that animals on low-protein diets have lower 136 energy efficiencies (Figure 4.1‘) and significantly higher metabolic rates per unit of metabolizable energy (Table 3.6) than highoprotein-fed animals. Results suggest that energy efficiencies in older animals are less affected by low dietary nitrogen than those in younger animals. Differential responses due to age may indicate that the DIT response is greater in younger, growing animals because they require more dietary protein to sustain growth. However, the results presented here are far from conclu- sive, since the older animals used in experiment 1 were not tested at the lower protein level (2.5 % casein) used to test the young weanlings in experiment 3. More research is needed to elucidate the relationship between low-protein DIT and the protein needs of fully grown adult animals. Voles required less dietary protein to stimulate DI’T than laboratory rats. The most likely explanation for this is that voles are better adapted at assimilating nutri— ents from a low-quality diet. This is probably due to selective pressures associated with herbivory that may have led to the evolution of high weight-specific BMRs in voles. Weight-specific metabolic rates for voles (experiment 3) were several times higher than those for rats in a similar study (Rothwell and Stock 1987c), indicating a high metabolic turnover rate for voles. Although the rats used by Rothwell and Stock (1987c) were much larger than the voles used in experiment 3, the differences in metabolic rates were higher than predicted based on weight differences alone. A high metabolic turnover rate could facilitate oxidation of excess food intake and increase the rate of uptake of essential nutrients such as dietary nitrogen by distillation from a ‘For convenience, a graphical summary of some of the quantitative data discussed in Chapter III is provided at the end of this chapter. 137 high bulk intake of low-nitrogen forage. Thus, herbivorous voles may possess greater intrinsic capabilities for oxidation of bulk, low-quality foodstuffs than omnivorous species such as rats. Data on the specific binding of GDP to brown fat mitochondria in voles (Figure 4.2) provides no evidence that BAT is involved in the DIT response to low protein intake observed in these animals. However, an elevated NST response, known to be mediated by BAT in small mammals, may have clouded these results. Therefore, more research is needed in this area before any conclusions can be made. Energy balance studies involving analysis of BAT should probably be conducted at temperatures just below thermoneutrality, to minimize NST and prevent the putative hyperthermic suppression of DIT. Prairie voles showed a definite hyperphagic response to low-protein diets, demonstrating that they can behaviorally change their intake in response to the nutri- tional quality of their food supply. For voles feeding on a 2.5 % casein diet in experiment 7, the rate of food consumption increased over the 21-day feeding trial for mean daily food consumption and weight-specific mean daily food consumption. Over the same period, mean daily food consumption and weight-specific mean daily food consumption declined in voles fed a 15% casein diet (Figures 3.8 and 4.5). However, gross food consumption for the first 10 days (total food consumed for 10 days) by the low-protein group showed no evidence of excess intake (Figure 4.6) and was statistically lower than the 10—day food intake recorded for the 15 % casein group (Table 3.7). Since voles consuming a 2.5% casein diet for 10 days showed a lower energy efficiency than voles feeding on a 15 % casein diet (Figure 4.1), yet consumed 138 less food during that period, it is unlikely that the low energy efficiency of the 2.5 % casein group was triggered by excess gross energy intake. It is more probable that the low energy efficiency observed in low-protein-fed voles was an acute response, possibly caused by an imbalance of nutrients in the diet due to the low ratio of protein to energy in the diet. For voles consuming low-protein diets, ‘excess’ energy consumption may be meal-based and result from the daily intake of a high-energy, low-protein diet. In this view, excess energy is energy that is present in the diet in surplus of what might be considered a ‘balanced diet’ for protein and energy content. It is possible that voles respond in the acute time frame and oxidize daily increments of excess energy ingested from an unbalanced diet, thus displaying a rapid metabolic response to acute variations in nutrient and energy intake. However, it is also possible that voles can respond within a chronic time frame to eliminate excess gross intake by DIT. This is evidenced by the steady increase in food intake over a 21-day period associated with only a moderate gain in weight (Table 3.9 and Figure 4.5). More lengthy and comprehensive studies of energy balance and food consumption are needed to differentiate between immediate and long-term thermogenic responses. The data for tissue changes in voles showed that animals on high-protein diets exhibited the greatest increases in protein, lipid, water, and other (Figure 4.7). Voles fed diets low in protein showed only a moderate increase in dry tissue. However, the relative increases in dry tissue components were the same for low-, medium-, and 139 high-protein groups, as were the relative proportions of tissue constituents in the final dry tissue mass (Tables 3.9-3.11 and Figure 4.8). Why the relative proportions of tissue constituents in the final dry tissue mass did not differ between dietary groups is not clear, but one possibility is that growth could be restricted to proportional increases in tissue deposition and may be dependent on the relative availability of nutrients in the diet. If such a constraint exists, then an ability to differentially extract limiting nutrients from an unbalanced diet could aid in sustaining growth. Moreover, if we accept the premise that there is a developmental limitation based on proportional growth, then a metabolic response like DIT would be a necessity for growing animals consuming unbalanced diets, and therefore is likely to have evolved by natural selection. Nitrogen balance data imply that low-protein-fed voles use dietary nitrogen more efficiently than other diet components. Gross nitrogen efficiencies were higher than total energy efficiencies for both 2.5 % and 5 % casein groups (Figures 4.1 and 4.3), although the difference between mean energy and nitrogen efficiencies is probably not significantly different for the 2.5 % casein group. Also, when dietary nitrogen is limited, a small increase in nitrogen content can substantially increase gross nitrogen efficiency. This suggests that a small hyperphagic response by voles to low-protein diets could result in a substantial increase in growth, since total protein intake increases with increased feeding. The 2.5 % casein group did not show rapid growth, but as they increased food intake, they did recover initial growth loss and even had a moderate net gain (Figure 4.4 and Table 3.9). A more lengthy experiment measuring nitrogen balance and food consumption is needed to see if hyperphagia is 140 persistent and can augment growth in body protein over an extended length of time. If hyperphagia and low energy efficiencies persist during long-term experiments, then excess gross energy consumption could be removed by oxidation, thus differentially enhancing nitrogen retention. In summary, nutritional studies of wild herbivores suggest that nitrogen is a limiting nutrient in natural diets. Since laboratory animals respond to low-protein diets by increasing metabolic heat production per unit of ME intake, it may be that this thermogenenic response (termed diet-induced thermogenesis or DIT) is adaptive for animals in the wild that normally consume foodstuffs low in protein. A possible adaptive function for DIT is that it may aid in the acquisition of essential nitrogen from a low—quality food supply. This could be accomplished by the oxidation of nonessential caloric intake resulting in a distillation of nitrogen from a diet low in essential nitrogen. The results of experiments using wild herbivorous voles confirm that energy efficiencies are reduced as dietary protein declines. Also, voles show higher metabolic turnover rates than laboratory rats consuming similar low-quality diets, suggesting a greater inherent capacity for responding to the deleterious effects of low-nutrient intake via heightened metabolic rates. Voles respond to low dietary protein by increasing food intake, reducing efficiency of energy retention, and differentially increasing nitrogen retention efficien- cy. Thus, the metabolic distillation of protein from a nutrient-poor diet by DIT appears to be a possible adaptive trait and could play a central role in the nutritional ecology of wild animals that normally consume diets low in nutrients such as protein. 141 8 .1 i i 4 .4 x til-Illa“ £5 —+— am (Ion! WY GAIN/II) 81. - q i ~ P . . b |+l . r - a , 1 7 7 a In. I' 1 o u: I ma I—ll m Figure 4-1- Efficiency 0f energy reten- Figure 4.2. GDP binding, expts. 4 and tion, expt. 3. Error bars are :1; SEM. 6. L = low protein, 25% casein; H = high protein, 15% casein; C = cold (22°C); W = warm (28°C). Error bars are :1: SEM. Wfi‘ hula-Ida .9!“ d 1. llllllllllll .mu ............ 19' Figure 4.4. Growth of weanling voles, expt. 7. Error bars are :1: SEM. Ls Figure 4.3. Gross nitrogen efficiencies, expt. 7 ((body N gain/dietary N intake) x 100). Error bars are :1; SEM. 142 ,- I. i 1 a. i 3 m I'. .. - ...... g”. I. h _- _ u -" E n- - i'r ‘ we: 1"" * $- 10 _ I mou- § N V .. ”I“ E - \-:-.\1:;. 5 ca 1 a 1 A lama S % I l I'mnu ’ I 1. § § :33: I“ . I” mm Figure 4.5. Mean daily food consump- tion, expt. 7. Figure 4.6. Total mean food intake per vole for 10 and 21 days during expt. 7. Error bars are i SEM. 5 l 1 l 4- - 3- - E“ ‘ a 1- - “'— ....... Ion-rein. ‘l” ‘ l'atarChaua rat-woma- _: I l 1 0mm 25. 5! I” 610!!! Figure 4.7. Data are for weight changes in tissue constituents over a 21-day feeding period during expt. 7. Error bars are :1; SEM. 143 ooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo 'l 0000000000000000000000000000000000000000000 ‘ .uu VVVVVVVVV.\VVVVVVVVVVVVVVVVVVN «- . p . , a u m m Boar run .3 a Figure 4. 8. Relative proportions of dry tissue constituents for voles at the end of expt. 7. Error bars are :1: SEM. LIST OF REFERENCES American Institute of Nutrition 1977. Report of the AIN Ad Hoc Committee on standards for nutritional studies. J. Nutr. , 107:1340—1348. Andrewartha, H.G. and L.C. Birch 1954. The Distribution and Abundance of Ani- f‘ mals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Andrzejewski, R. 1975. Supplementary food and the winter dynamics of bank vole populations. Acta Theriol. , 20:23-40. Applebaum, S.W. and Y. Birk 1979. Saponins. In Herbivores: Their Interaction with Secondaty Plant Metabolites, pp. 539-566, eds. G.A. Rosenthal and DH. J anzen. New York: Academic Press. Arditi, R., L.R. Ginzburg, and HR. Akcakaya 1991. Variation in plankton densities among lakes: a case for ratio-dependent predation models. Am. Nat. , 138(5): 1287-1296. Armitage, G., G.R. Hervey, B.J. Rolls, E.A. Rowe, and G. Tobin 1981. Energy balance in young ‘cafeteria’-fed rats. J. Physiol., 317:48P—49P. Ashwell, M., S. Holt, G. Jennings, D.M. Stirling, P. Trayhum, and DA. York 1985 . Measurement by radioimmunoassay of the mitochondrial uncoupling protein from brown adipose tissue of obese (ob/ ob) mice and Zucker (fa/ fa) rats of different ages. FEBS Lett. , 179(2):233-237. Baker, R.H. 1983. Michigan Mammals. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press. Banach, K. 1986. The effect of increased food supply on the body growth rate and survival of bank voles in an island population. Acta Theriol. , 31:45-54. Barbosa, P. and D.K. Letoumeau, eds. , 1988. Novel Aspects of Insect—Plant Interac- tions. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Barlow, CA. and RA. Randolph 1978. Quality and quantity of plant sap available to the pea aphid. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am, 71:46—48. 144 145 Barr, H.G. and K.J. McCracken 1982. Absence of ‘diet-induced thermogenesis’ in growing rats kept at 29° and offered a varied diet. Proc. Nutr. Soc. , 41:63A. Barry, T.N., T.R. Manley, and 8.]. Duncan 1986. The role of condensed tannins in the nutritional value of Lotus pedunculatus for sheep. 4. Sites of carbohydrate and protein digestion as influenced by dietary reactive tannin concentration. Brit. J. Nutr., 55:123-137. Batzli, GO. and F .R. Cole 1979. Nutritional ecology of microtine rodents: digestibil- ity of forage. J. Mamm., 60:740-750. Batzli, GO. 1985. Nutrition. In Biology of New World Microtus, pp. 779-811, ed. R.H. Tamarin. Am. Soc. Mamm. Spec. Pub. No. 8. Bell, EA. 1978. Toxins in swds. In Biochemical Aspects of Plant and Animal Coevolution, pp. 143-161, ed. J .B. Harbome. London: Academic Press. Bell, R.H.V. 1970. The use of the herb layer by grazing ungulates in the Serengeti. In Animal Populations in Relation to Their Food Resources, pp. 111-124, ed. A. Watson. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Bell, R.H.V. 1971. A grazing system in the Serengeti. Scientific American, 225:86- 93. Belovsky, G.E. and 0.]. Schmitz 1991. Mammalian herbivore optimal foraging and the role of plant defenses. In Plant Defenses Against Mammalian Herbivory, pp. 1-28, eds. R.T. Palo and CT. Robbins. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Benz, G. 1974. Negative Ruckkoppelung durch Raum- und Nahrungskonkurrenz sowie Zyklische Veranderung der Nahrungsgrundlage als Regelprinzip in der Populationsdynamic des Gruen Larchenwicklers, Zeriaphera diniana (Guenee) (Lep. Tortricidae). Z. Angew. Entomol. , 76:196-228. Benz, G. 1977. Insect-induced resistance as a means of self defense in plants. Eucatp- ia/IOBC Work. Group Breed. Resistance Insects Mites, Bull, SROP, 1977/1978, pp. 155-159. Berenbaum, M. 1980. Adaptive significance of midgut pH in larval Lepidoptera. Am. Nat., 115:138-146. Berenbaum, M. 1981 . Patterns of furanocoumarin distribution and insect herbivory in the Umbelliferae: plant chemistry and community structure. Ecology, 63: 1254- 1266. 146 Bergeron, J .M. and L. Jodoin 1987. Defining “high quality” food resources of herbi- vores: the case for meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Oecologia, 71:510-517. Bergeron, J.M. and L. Jodoin 1989. Patterns of resource use, food quality, and health status of voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) trapped from fluctuating populations. Oecologia, 79:306-314. Bergeron, J .M., L. Jodoin, and Y. Jean 1987. Pathology of voles (Microtus pennsyl- vanicus) fed with plant extracts. J. Mamm., 68:73-79. Bemays, E.A., G.C. Driver, and M. Bilgener 1989. Herbivores and plant tannins. In Advances in Ecological Research, vol. 19, pp. 263-302, eds. M. Begon, A.H. Fitter, E.D. Ford, and A. Macfadyen. London: Academic Press. Bestley, J.W., P.N. Bramley, P.M.S. Dobson, A. Mahanty, and G. Tobin 1982. Energy balance in ‘cafeteria’-fed young ‘Charles River’ Sprague-Dawley rats. J. Physiol. , 330:7OP-71P. Bligh, J. and K.G. Johnson 1973. Glossary of terms for thermal physiology. J. Applied Physiol. , 35(6):94l-961. Boutin, S. 1984. Effect of late winter food addition on numbers and movements of snowshoe hares. Oecologia, 62:393-400. Boutin, S. 1990. Food supplementation experiments with terrestrial vertebrates: patterns, problems, and the future. Can. J. Zool., 68:203-220. Brooks, S.L., N.J. Rothwell, and MJ. Stock 1980. Increased proton conductance pathway in brown adipose tissue mitochondria of rats exhibiting diet-induced thermogenesis. Nature, 286:274-276. Bryant, J .P. 1981. Phytochemical deterrence of snowshoe hare browsing by adventi- tious shoots of four Alaskan trees. Science, 213:889-890. Bryant, J .P., F.S. Chapin, and DR. Klein 1983. Carbon/nutrient balance of boreal plants in relation to vertebrate herbivory. Oikos, 40:357-368. Bryant, J .P., F.D. Provenza, J. Pastor, P.B. Reichardt, T.P. Clausen, and LT. du Toit 1991. Interactions between woody plants and browsing mammals mediat- ed by secondary metabolites. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 22:431-446. Bryant, J .P., G.D. Wieland, T. Clausen, and P. Kuropat 1985. Interactions of snowshoe hare and feltleaf willow in Alaska. Ecology, 66(5): 1564-1573. 147 Buchsbaum, R., J. Wilson, and I. Valiela 1986. Digestibility of plant constituents by Canada geese and Atlantic brant. Ecology, 67(2):386—393. Bucyanayandi, J .D. and J.M. Bergeron 1990. Effects of food quality on feeding patterns of meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) along a community gradient. J. Mamm., 71(3):390-396. Bukowiecki, L.J . 1986. Lipid metabolism in brown adipose tissue. In Brown Adipose Tissue, pp. 105-121, eds. P. Trayhum and D.G. Nicholls. London: Edward Arnold. Cannon, B. and O. Lindberg 1979. Mitochondria from brown adipose tissue: isolation and properties. Methods Enzymol. , 55:65-78. Cates, R.G. 1980. Feeding patterns of monophagous, oligophagous and polyphagous insect herbivores: the effect of resource abundance and plant chemistry. Oecologia, 46:22-31. Cates, R.G. and DP. Rhoades 1977. Patterns in the production of antiherbivore chemical defenses in plant communities. Biochem. Syst. Ecol. , 5: 185-193. Chapin, F.S., III 1980. The mineral nutrition of wild plants. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. , 11:233-260. Chiang, H., D.M. Norris, A. Ciepiela, P. Shapiro, and A. Oosterwyk 1987. Induc- ible versus constitutive PI 227687 soybean resistance to Mexican bean beetle, Epilachna varivestis. J. Chem. Ecol., 13(4):741-750. Clausen, T.P., P.B. Reichardt, J.P. Bryant, R.A. Werner, K. Post, and K. Frisby 1989. Chemical model for short-term induction in quaking aspen (Populus tremuloides) foliage against herbivores. J. Chem. Ecol. , 15(9):2335-2346. Coe, M.J., D.H. Cumming, and J. Phillipson 1976. Biomass and production of large African herbivores in relation to rainfall and primary production. Oecologia, 22:341-354. Cole, RR. and G.C. Batzli 1978. Influence of supplemental fwding on a vole p0pulation. J. Mamm., 59:809-819. Cole, RR. and G.C. Batzli. 1979. Nutrition and population dynamics of the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster, in central Illinois. J. Anim. Ecol. , 48:455-470. Coley, P.D., J.P. Bryant, and RS. Chapin 1985. Resource availability and plant antiherbivore defense. Science, 230:895-899. 148 Cooper, S.M. and N. Owen-Smith 1986. Effects of plant Spinescence on large mammalian herbivores. Oecologia, 68:446-455. Cooper-Driver, G., S. Finch, and T. Swain 1977. Seasonal variation in secondary plant compounds in relation to palatability of Pteridium aquilinum. Biochem. Syst. Ecol., 5:177-183. Crompton, A.W., CR. Taylor, and J .A. Jagger 1978. Evolution of homeothermy in mammals. Nature, 272:333-336. Demment, M.W. and R]. Van Soest 1985. A nutritional explanation for body-size patterns of ruminant and nonruminant herbivores. Am. Nat. ,125(5):641-672. Denno, RF. and M.S. McClure, eds., 1983. Variable Plants and Herbivores in Natural and Managed Systems. New York: Academic Press. Desautels, M. , G. Zaror-Behrens, and J. Himms-Hagen 1978. Increased purine nucleotide binding, altered popypeptide composition, and thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue mitochondria of cold-acclimated rats. Can. J. Biochem. , 56:378-383. Desy, EA. and GO. Batzli 1989. Effects of food availability and predation on prairie vole demography: a field experiment. Ecology, 70(2):411-421. Desy, EA. and C.F. Thompson 1983. Effects of supplemental food on a Microtus pennsylvanicus population in central Illinois. J. Anim. Ecol. , 52: 127-140. Donald, P., G.C. Pitts, and S.L. Pohl 1981. Body weight and composition in labora- tory rats: effects of diets with high or low protein concentrations. Science, 211:185-186. Edozien, LC. and B.R. Switzer 1978. Influence of diet on growth in the rat. J. Nutr., 108:282-290. Ehrlich, RR. and L.C. Birch 1967. The “balance of nature” and “population con- trol.” Am. Nat., 101:97-107. Elton, C. 1936. Animal Ecology. New York: MacMillan. Evans, D.M. 1973. Seasonal variations in the body composition and nutrition of the vole Microtus agrestis., J. Anim. Ecol. , 42:1-18. Falcou, R., F. Bouillaud, G. Mory, M. Apfelbaum, and D. Ricquier 1985. Increase of uncoupling protein and its mRNA in brown adipose tissue of rats fed on ‘cafeteria diet’. Biochem. J., 231:241-244. 149 Feeny, P. 1970. Seasonal changes in oak leaf tannins and nutrients as a cause of spring feeding by winter moth caterpillars. Ecology, 51:565-581. Feeny, P. 1976. Plant apparency and chemical defense. In Recent Advances in Phytochemistry, vol. 10, pp. 1-40, eds. J.W. Wallace and R.L. Mansell. New York: Plenum Press. Felsenstein, J. 1985. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am. Nat. , 125:1-15. Ford, R.G. and F.A. Pitelka 1984. Resource limitation in populations of the Califor- nia vole. Ecology, 65:122-136. Foster, D.G. and M.L. Frydman 1978. Nonshivering thermogenesis in the rat. 11. Measurements of blood flow with microspheres point to brown adipose tissue as the dominant site of calorigenesis induced by noradrenaline. Can. J. Physiol. Phannacol. , 56:1 10-122. Foster, DD. and S.W.Y. Ma 1989. The effector of diet—induced thermogenesis: brown adipose tissue of liver? In Hormones, Thermogenesis, and Obesity, pp. 165-171, eds. H. Lardy and F. Stratrnan. New York: Elsevier. Fox, LR. 1981. Defense dynamics in plant-herbivore systems. Amer. Zool. , 21:853- 864. Fraenkel, G. 1959. The raison d’etre of secondary plant substances. Science, 129: 1466-1470. Freeland, W.J., P.H. Calcott and LR. Anderson 1985. Tannins and saponin: interac- tion in herbivore diets. Biochem. Syst. Ecol. , 13(2): 189-193. Freeland, W.J. and DH. Janzen 1974. Strategies in herbivory by mammals: the role of plant secondary compounds. Am. Nat. , 108:269-289. Fretwell, SD. 1977. The regulation of plant communities by the food chains exploit- ing them. Perspect. Biol. Med., 20:169-185. Fretwell, SD. 1987. Food chain dynamics: the central theory of ecology? Oikos, 50:291-301. Fuller, M.F. 1983. Energy and nitrogen balances in young pigs maintained at con- stant weight with diets of differing protein content. J. Nutr. , 113:15-20. Futuyma, DJ. 1976. Food plant specialization and environmental predictability in Lepidoptera. Am. Nat., 110:285-292. 150 Gardarsson, A. and R. Moss 1970. Selection of food by Icelandic ptarmigan in relation to its availability and nutritive value. In Animal Populations In Relation To Their Food Resources, pp. 47-71, ed. A. Watson, Oxford: Black- well Scientific Publications. Garrow, J.S. 1978. Energy Balance and Obesity in Man. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Gettinger, RD. 1984. Seasonal patterns of nitrogen utilization by pocket gophers, Thomomys bottae. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 78A(4):657—659. Gilbert, BS. and OJ. Krebs 1981. Effects of extra food on Peromyscus and Clethri- onomys populations in the southern Yukon. Oecologia, 51:326-331. Glick, Z. 1987. The thermic effect of a meal. J. Obesity Wt. Reg., 6:170—178. Glick, Z. and M.A. J oslyn 1970. Food intake depression and other metabolic effects of tannic aci in the rat. J. Nutr. , 100:509—515. Glick, Z., R]. Teague, and G.A. Bray 1981. Brown adipose tissue: thermic response increased by a single low protein, carbohydrate meal. Science, 213: 1 125-1127. Goldberg, J .C. and B.L.G. Morgan 1983. The response of brown adipose tissue (BAT) to cafeteria feeding in the Zucker rat. Fed. Proc. , 42(5): 1189. Goldberg, M., N.R. Tabroff, and R.H. Tamarin 1980. Nutrient variation in beach grass in relation to beach vole feeding. Ecology, 61(5): 1029-1033. Gould, SJ. and RC. Lewontin. 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the Pan- glossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 205:581-598. Grant, S. 1984. Beauty and the Beast: The Coevolution of Plants and Animals. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons. Gross, LE, 2. Wang, and BA. Wunder 1985. Effects of food quality and energy nwds: changes in gut morphology and capacity of Microtus ochrogaster. J. Mamm., 66(4):661-667. Gulick, A. 1922. A study of weight regulation in the adult human body during over- nutrition. Am. J. Physiol., 60:371-395. Gurr, M.J., R. Mawson, N.J. Rothwell, and MJ. Stock 1980. Effects of manipulat- ing dietary protein and energy intake on energy balance and thermogenesis in the pig. J. Nutr., 110:532-542. 151 Hairston, G.N., F.E. Smith, and LB. Slobodkin 1960. Community structure, population control, and competition. Am. Nat. , 94:421-425. Hamilton, T.S. 1939a. The heat increments of diets balanced and unbalanced with respect to protein. J. Nutr., 17:583-599. Hamilton, T.S. 1939b. The growth, activity, and composition of rats fed diets balanced and unbalanced with respect to protein. J. Nutr. , 17:565-582. Hamilton, W.J., Jr. 1941. The food of small forest mammals in eastern United States. J. Mamm., 22(3):250—263. Hammond, K.A. and BA. Wunder 1991. The role of diet quality and energy need in the nutritional ecology of a small herbivore, Microtus ochrogaster. Physiol. Zool., 64(2):541-567. Hanhimaeki, S. 1989. Induced resistance in mountain birch: defence against leaf- chewing insect guild and herbivore competition. Oecologia, 81(2):242-248. Hanley, T.A., C.T. Robbins, A.E. Hagerman, and C. McArthur 1992. Predicting digestible protein and digestible dry matter in tannin-containing forages consumed by ruminants. Ecology, 73(2):537-541. Hayssen, V. and RC. Lacy 1985. Basal metabolic rates in mammals: taxonomic differences in the allometry of BMR and body mass. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., 81A:741-754. Heldmaier, G. 1989. Seasonal acclimatization of energy requirements in mammals: functional significance of body weight control, hypothermia, torpor and hibernation. In Energy Transformations in Cells and Organisms, Proceedings of the 10th Conference of the European Society for Comparative Physiology and Biochemistry, pp. 130-139, eds. W. Wieser and E. Gnaiger. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag. Helrich, K. , ed., 1990. Ofi‘icial Methods of Analysis of the Association of Ofiicial Analytical Chemists. Arlington: Association of Official Analytical Chemists. Henderson, QB. 1990. The influence of seed apparency, nutrient content and chemi- cal defenses on dietary preference in Dipodomys ordii. Oecologia, 82:333-341. Hervey, G.R. and G. Tobin 1981. Brown adipose tissue and diet-induced therrnogene- sis. Nature, 289:699. Hervey, G.R. and G. Tobin 1982. The part played by variation of energy expenditure in the regulation of energy balance. Proc. Nutr. Soc. , 41: 137-153. 152 Hervey, G.R. and G. Tobin 1983. Luxuskonsumption, diet-induced thermogenesis and brown fat: a critical review. Clin. Science, 64:7-18. Hill, R.W. 1975. Daily torpor in Peromyscus leucopus on an adequate diet. Comp. Biochem. Physiol., 51A:413-423. Hill, R.W. and G.A. Wyse. 1989. Animal Physiology. Harper and Row: New York. Himms-Hagen, J. 1986. Cold- versus diet-induced thermogenesis in brown adipose tissue: different strategies in different species. In Living in the Cold, pp. 93- 100, eds. H.C. Heller, X.J. Musacchia and L.C.H. Wang. New York: Elsevier. Himms—Hagen, J. 1989. Role of thermogenesis in the regulation of energy balance in relation to obesity. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. , 67:394-401. Himms-Hagen, J. 1990. Brown adipose tissue thermogenesis: interdisciplinary studies. FASEB J. , 4:2890-2898. Himms-Hagen J. and M. Desautels 1978. A mitochondrial defect in brown adipose tissue of the obese (ob/ob) mouse: reduced binding of purine nucleotides and a failure to respond to cold by an increase in binding. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. , 83(2):628-634. I-Iimms-Hagen, J ., J. Triandafillou, and C. Gwilliam 1981. Brown adipose tissue of cafeteria-fed rats. Am. J. Physiol., 241(4):E116-E120. Hladik, A. 1978. Phenology of leaf production in rain forest of Gabon: distribution and composition of food for folivores. In The Ecology of Arboreal Folivores, pp. 51-71, ed. G.G. Montgomery. Washington: Smithsonian. Hoffmann, RS. and J.W. Koeppl 1985. Zoogeography. In Biology of New World Microtus, pp. 84-115, ed. R.H. Tamarin. Am. Soc. Mamm. Spec. Pub. No. 8. Hogan, S., D.V. Coscina, and J. Himms-Hagen 1982. Brown adipose tissue of rats with obesity-inducing ventromedial hypothalamic lesions. Am. J. Physiol. , 243(6):E338-E344. Horsefield, D. 1977. Relationship between feeding of Philaenus spumarius (L.) and the amino acid concentration in the xylem sap. Ecol. Entomol. , 2:259-266. Howe, HF. and L.C. Westley 1988. Ecological Relationships of Plants and Animals. New York: Oxford University Press. 153 Hulbert, A.J. and T.J. Dawson 1974. Standard metabolism and body temperature of perameloid marsupials from different environments. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. , 47Az583-590. Ikeda, T., F. Matsumura, and D.M. Benjamin 1977. Mechanisms of feeding discrimi- nation between matured and juvenile foliage by two species of pine sawflies. J. Chem. Ecol. , 3:677-694. Janis, C. 1976. The evolutionary strategy of the Equidae and the origins of rumen and cecal digestion. Evolution, 30:757—774. Jansky, L. 1973. Non-shivering thermogenesis and its thermoregulatory significance. Biol.Rev., 48:85-132. Janzen, D.H. 1978. Complications in interpreting the chemical defenses of trees against tropical arboreal plant-eating vertebrates. In The Ecology of Arboreal Folivores, pp. 73-84, ed. G.G. Montgomery. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. J effries, M. 1990. Evidence of induced plant defences in a pondweed. Freshwat. Biol. , 23(2):265-269. Jones, D.I.H. and AD. Wilson 1987. Nutritive quality of forage. In The Nutrition of Herbivores, pp. 87-89, eds. J.B. Hacker and J.H. Ternouth. Orlando: Aca- demic Press. Jung, H.G. and GO. Batzli 1981. Nutritional ecology of microtine rodents: effects of plant extracts on the growth of arctic microtines. J. Mamm. , 62(2):286-292. Karasov, W.H. 1982. Energy assimilation, nitrogen requirement, and diet in free- living antelope ground squirrels, Arnmospennophilus leucurus. Physiol. 2001. , 55:378-392. Karasov, W.H. 1985 . Nutrient constraints in the feeding ecology of an omnivore in a seasonal environment. Oecologia, 66:280-290. Karban, R. 1987. Environmental conditions affecting the strength of induced resis- tance against mites in cotton. Oecologia, 73(3):414-419. Kendall, W.A. and K.T. Leath 1976. Effect of saponins on palatability of alfalfa the meadow voles. Agron. J., 68:473-476. Kendall, W.A. and RT. Sherwood 1975. Palatability of leaves of tall fescue and reed canarygrass and some of their alkaloids to meadow voles. Agron. J. , 67 :667- 671. 154 Kevonian, A.V., J.G. Vander Tuig and DR. Romsos 1984. Consumption of a low protein diet increases norepinephrine turnover in brown adipose tissue of adult rats. J. Nutr., 114:543-549. King, LR. and M.F. Murphy 1985. Periods of nutritional stress in the annual cycles of endotherms: fact or fiction? Amer. Zool. , 25:955-964. Klaus, S., G. Heldmaier, and D. Ricquier 1988. Seasonal acclimation of bank voles and wood mice: nonshivering thermogenic properties of brown adipose tissue mitochondria. J. Comp. Physiol. , 158: 157-164. Kleiber, M. 1932. Body size and metabolism. Hilgardia, 6:315-353. Kleiber, M. 1945 . Dietary deficiencies and energy metabolism. Nutr. Abstr. Rev. , 15:207-222. Kleiber, M. 1947. Body size and metabolic rate. Physiol. Rev. , 27:511-541. Kleiber, M. 1975a. The Fire of Life: an Introduction to Animal Energetics. Hunting- ton: Krieger. Kleiber, M. 1975b. Metabolic turnover rate: a physiological meaning of the metabolic rate per unit body weight. J. theor. Biol. , 53:199-204. Klein, DR. 1970. Food selection by North American deer and their response to over- utilization of preferred plant species. In Animal Populations In Relation To Their Food Resources, pp. 25-46, ed. A. Watson. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Kopecky, J ., L. Sigurdson, I.R.A. Park, and J. I-Iimms-Hagen 1986. Thyroxine 5’- deiodinase in hamster and rat brown adipose tissue: effect of cold and diet. Am. J. Physiol., 251(14):E1-E7. Krebs, CJ. and K.T. DeLong 1965. A Microtus population with supplemental food. J. Mamm., 46:566-573. Krebs, C.J., S. Boutin, and BS. Gilbert 1986. A natural feeding experiment on a declining snowshoe hare population. Oecologia, 70: 194-197. Krebs, C.J., B.S. Gilbert, S. Boutin, A.R.E. Sinclair, and J.N.M. Smith 1986. Population biology of snowshoe hares. I. Demography of food-supplemented populations in the southern Yukon, 1976-84. J. Anim. Ecol., 55:963-982. Krebs, J .R. and NB. Davies 1987. An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates. 155 Landsberg, L. and LB. Young 1983. Autonomic regulation of thermogenesis. In Mammalian Thermogenesis, pp. 99-140, eds. L. Girardier and MJ. Stock. London: Chapman and Hall. Leather, S.R., A.D. Watt, and 6.1. Forrest 1987. Insect-induced cherrrical changes in young lodgepole pine (Pinus contorta): the effect of previous defoliation on oviposition, growth and survival of the pine beauty moth, Panolis flammea. Ecol. Entomol. , 12(3):275-281. Lin, H., M. Kogan, and D. Fischer 1990. Induced resistance in soybean to the Mexican bean beetle (Coleoptera: Coccinellidae): comparisons of inducing factors. Environ. Entomol. , 19(6):1852-1857. Lindroth, R.L. 1988. Adaptations of mammalian herbivores to plant chemical defens- es. In Chemical Mediation of Coevolution, pp. 415-445, ed. K.C. Spencer. San Diego: Academic Press. Lindroth, R.L. 1989. Mammalian herbivore-plant interactions. In Plant-Animal Interactions, pp. 123-206, ed. W.G. Abrahamson. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lindroth, R.L. 1991. Differential toxicity of plant allelochemicals to insects: roles of enzymatic detoxication systems. In Insect-Plant Interactions, pp. 1-33, ed. E. Bemays. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Lindroth, R.L. and GO. Batzli 1983. Detoxification of some naturally occurring phenolics by prairie voles: a rapid assay of glucuronidation metabolism. Biochem. Syst. Ecol. , 11:405-409. Lindroth, R.L. and GO. Batzli 1984a. Food habits of the meadow vole (Microtus pennsylvanicus) in bluegrass and prairie habitats. J. Mamm. , 65(4):600-606. Lindroth, R.L. and GO. Batzli 1984b. Plant phenolics as chemical defenses: effects of natural phenolics on survival and growth of prairie voles (Microtus ochro- gaster). J. Chem. Ecol., 10:229-244. Lindroth, R.L. and GO. Batzli 1986. Inducible plant chemical defenses: a cause of vole population cycles? J. Anim. Ecol. , 55:431-449. Lindroth, R.L., G.O. Batzli, and SJ. Avildsen 1986. Lespedeza phenolics and Penstemon alkaloids: effects on digestion and growth of voles. J. Chem. Ecol. , 12:713-728. Ma, S.W.Y. and DO. Foster 1989. Brown adipose tissue, liver, and diet-induced thermogenesis in cafateria diet-fed rats. Can. J. Physiol. Phannacol. , 67 :376- 381. 156 Markwell, M.A.K., S.M. Haas, N.E. Tolbert, and LL. Bieber 1981. Protein determination in membrane and lipoprotein samples: manual and automated procedures. Methods Enzymol. , 72:296-303. Martin, ER 1956. A population study of the prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) in northeastern Kansas. Univ. Kansas Pub. Mus. Nat. Hist, 8(6):361-416. Mattson, W.J., Jr. 1980. Herbivory in relation to plant nitrogen content. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 11:119-161. Mayr, E. 1983. How to carry out the adaptationist program? Am. Nat., 121(3):324- 334. McArthur, C., A.E. Hagerman, and CT. Robbins 1991. Physiological strategies of mammalian herbivores against plant defenses. In Plant Defenses Against Mammalian Herbivory, pp. 103-131, eds. R.T. Palo and CT. Robbins. Boca Raton: CRC Press. McBee, R.H. 1971. Significance of intestinal microflora in herbivory. Ann. Rev. Ecol., 2:165-176. McCracken, K.J. 1975a. The effect of overfeeding on normal adult rats. Proc. Nutr. Soc., 34:15A-16A. McCracken, K.J. 1975b. Effect of feeding pattern on the energy metabolism of rats given low-protein diets. Br. J. Nutr. , 33:277-289. McCracken, K.J . 1976. A futile energy cycle in adult rats given a low-protein diet at high levels of energy intake? Proc. Nutr. Soc. , 35:59A. McCracken, K.J. and A. McAllister 1984. Energy metabolism and body composition of young pigs given low-protein diets. Br. J. Nutr., 51:225-234. McCracken, K.J. and HI. Barr 1982. Energy balance and body fat changes in young ‘cafeteria’-fed rats kept at 24°C. J. Physiol., 330:69P-70P. McCracken, K.J. and H.G. Barr 1985. Reply to letter by Rothwell and Stock. Br. J. Nutr., 53:192. McKey, D. 1979. The distribution of secondary compounds within plants. In Herbi- vores: Their Interaction with Secondary Plant Metabolites, pp. 56-134, eds. G.A. Rosenthal and D.H. Janzen. New York: Academic Press. McNab, B.K. 1963. A model of the energy budget of a wild mouse. Ecology, 44(3):521-532. 157 McNab, BK. 197 8. Energetics of arboreal folivores: physiological problems and ecological consequences of fading on an ubiquitous food supply. In The Ecology of Arboreal Folivores, pp. 153-162, ed. G.G. Montgomery. Washing- ton, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. McNab, B.K. 1980. Food habits, energetics, and the population biology of mammals. Am. Nat., 116(1):106-124. McNab, B.K. 1982. Evolutionary alternatives in the physiological ecology of bats. In Ecology of Bats, pp. 151-200, ed. T.H. Kunz. New York: Plenum Press. McNab, B.K. 1986. The influence of food habits on the energetics of eutherian mammals. Ecolog. Monog., 56(1):1-19. McNab, B.K. 1992. Energy expenditure: a short history. In Mammalian Energetics. Interdisciplinary Views of Metabolism and Reproduction, pp. 1-15. , eds. T.E. Tomasi and T.H. Horton. Ithaca: Comstock Publishing Associates. McNab, B.K. and P.R. Morrison 1963. Body temperature and metabolism in subspe- cies of Peronryscus from arid and mesic environments. Ecol. Monogr. , 33:63- 82. McNaughton, 8.]. 1987. Adaptation of herbivores to seasonal changes in nutrient supply. In The Nutrition of Herbivores, pp. 391-408, eds. J.B. Hacker and J.H. Ternouth. Orlando: Academic Press. McNaughton, S.J., M. Oesterheld, D.A. Frank, and K.J. Williams 1989. Ecosystem level patterns of primary productivity and herbivory in terrestrial habitats. Nature, 341:142-144. McNaughton, S.J., J.L. Tarrants, M.M. McNaughton, and R.H. Davis 1985. Silica as a defense against herbivory and a growth promoter in African grasses. Ecology, 66:528-535. McNeil, S. and T.R.E. Southwood 1978. The role of nitrogen in the development of insect/plant relationships. In Biochemical Aspects of Plant and Animal Coevolution, pp. 77-98, ed. J.B. Harbome. London: Academic. Meyer, M.W. and W.H. Karasov 1989. There is no simple dichotomy between digestibility-reduction and toxicity in action of plant secondary chemicals. Bull. Ecol. Soc. Amer. , 70:203. Milewski, A.V., T.P. Young, and D. Madden 1991. Thorns as induced defenses: experimental evidence. Oecologia, 86(1):?0—75. 158 Miller, BS, and P.R. Payne 1962. Weight maintenance and food intake. J. Nutr., 78:255-262. Milton, K. 1979. Factors influencing leaf choice by howler monkeys: a test of some hypothesis of food selection by generalist herbivores. Am. Not. , 114(3):362- 378. Mitchell, H.H. 1934. Balanced diets, net energy value and specific dynamic effects. Science, 80:558-561. Mittler, T.E. 1953. Amino-acids in phloem sap and their excretion by aphids. Nature, 172:207. Moen, J. and L. Oksanen 1991. Ecosystem trends. Nature, 353:510. Mole, S., L.G. Butler, and G. Iason 1990a. Defense against dietary tannin in herbi- vores: a survey for proline rich salivary proteins in mammals. Biochem. Syst. Ecol., 18:287-293. Mole, S. J.C. Rogler, C.J. Morel], and LG. Butler 1990b. Herbivore growth reduction by tannins: use of Waldbauer ratio techniques and manipulation of salivary protein production to elucidate mechanisms of action. Biochem. Syst. Ecol., 18:183-197. Moore, B.J . 1987. The cafeteria diet—an inappropriate tool for studies of therrnogen- esis. J. Nutr., 117:227-231. Morrison, D.W. 1978. Foraging ecology and energetics of the frugivorous bat, Artibeus jarnaicensis. Ecology, 59:716-723. Murdoch, W.M. 1966. “Community structure, population control, and competi- tion”—a critique. Am. Nat., 100:219-226. Myers, J .H. 1988. The induced defense hypothesis: does it apply to the population dynamics of insects? In Chemical Mediation of Coevolution, pp. 345-365, ed. K. Spencer. San Diego: Academic Press. Nedergaard, J., A. Raasmaja, and B. Cannon 1984. Parallel increases in amount of (3H)GDP binding and thermogenin antigen in brown-adipose-tissue mitochon- dria of cafeteria-fed rats. Biochem. biophys. Res. Commun. , 122(3): 1328- 1336. Negus, N .C. and PJ. Berger 1977. Experimental triggering of reproduction in a natural population of Microtus montanus. Science, 196: 1230-1231. 159 Neumann, R.O. 1902. Experimentelle Beitrage Zur Iehre von dem taglichen Nahr- ungsbedarf des menschen unter besonderer Berucksichtigung der notwendigen Eiweissmenge. Arch. Hyg., 45:1-87. N icholls, D. 1976. Hamster brown-adipose-tissue mitochondria. Purine nucleotide control of the ion conductance of the inner membrane, the nature of the nucleotide binding site. Eur. J. Biochem. , 62:223-228. Nicholls, DJ. 1979. Brown adipose tissue mitochondria. Biochim. biophys. Acta. , 549: 1-29. Nicholls, D. and E. Rial 1988. The function of the uncoupling protein in the intact brown fat cell. In Integration of Mitochondrial Function, pp. 517-526, eds. J .J . Lemasters, C. Hackenbrock, R. Thurman, and H. Westerhoff. New York: Plenum Press. Nicholson, A.J. 1933. The balance of animal populations. J. Anim. Ecol., 2:132-178. Nowak, RM. 1991. Walker’s Mammals of the World. Baltimore: John Hopkins. Ohmart, GP. and RB. Edwards 1991. Insect herbivory on Eucalyptus. Ann. Rev. Entomol., 36:637-657. Oksanen, L. 1988. Ecosystem organization: mutualism and cybemetics or plain Darwinian struggle for existence? Am. Nat., 131:424-444. Oksanen, L., F.D. Fretwell, J. Arruda, and P. Niemela 1981. Exploitation ecosys- tems in gradients of primary productivity. Am. Nat. , 118:240-261. Oksanen, L. and T. Oksanen 1981. Lemmings (Lemmas lemmas) and gray-sided voles (Clethrionomys rufits) in interaction with their resources and predators on Finnmarks vidda, northern Norway. Rep. Kevo Subarct. Res. Stat. , 17:7-31. Palo, RT. 1985. Chemical defense in birch: inhibition of digestibility in ruminants by phenolic extracts. Oecologia, 68: 10-14. Palo, RT. and Robbins, C.T., eds., 1991. Plant Defenses Against Mammalian Herbivory. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Parra, R. 1978. Comparison of foregut and hindgut fermentation in herbivores. In The Ecology of Arboreal Folivores, pp. 205-229, ed. G.G. Montgomery. Washington: Smithsonian. Pearson, OR 1954. The daily energy requirements of a wild anna hummingbird. Condor, 56(6):317-322. 160 Petrusewicz, K. and A. Macfadyen 1970. Productivity of Terrestrial Animals: Princi- ples and Methods. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Phillipson, J. 1975. Rainfall, primary production and ‘carrying capacity’ of Tsavo National Park (East), Kenya. E. Afr. erdl. J., 13:171-201. Raup, MJ. and RF. Denno 1983. Leaf age as a predictor of herbivore distribution and abundance. In Variable Plants and Herbivores in Natural and Managed Systems, pp. 91-124, eds. R.F. Denno and M.S. McClure. New York: Academic Press. Rayor, LS. 1985. Effects of habitat quality on growth, age of first reproduction, and dispersal in Gunnison’s prairie dogs (Cynomys gunnisont). Can. J. Zool. , 63:2835-2840. Reese, LC. and SD. Beck 1978. Inter-relationships of nutritional indices and dietary moisture in the black cutworm (Agrotis ipsilon) digestive efficiency. J. Insect Physiol. , 24:473-479. Reichardt, P.B., J.P. Bryant, T.P Clausen, and GD. Wieland 1984. Defense of winter-dormant Alaska paper birch against snowshoe hares. Oecologia, 65:58- 69. Rhoades, D.F. 1983. Herbivore population dynamics and plant chemistry. In Variable Plants and Herbivores in Natural and Managed Systems, pp. 155-220, eds. R.F. Demo and M.S. McClure. New York: Academic Press. Rhoades, D.F. 1985 . Offensive-defensive interactions between herbivores and plants: their relevance in herbivore population dynamics and ecological theory. Am. Nat., 125:205-238. Rhoades, D.F. and R.G. Cates 1976. Toward a general theory of plant antiherbivore chemistry. In Recent Advances in Phytochemistry, vol. 10, pp. 168-213, eds. J.W. Wallace and R.L. Mansell. New York: Plenum Press. Ricklefs, R.E. and K.K. Matthew 1982. Chemical characteristics of foliage of some deciduous trees in southeastern Ontario. Can. J. Bot. , 60:2037-2045. Robbins, C.T., A.E. Hagerman, P.J. Austin, C. McArthur, and T.A. Hanley 1991. Variation in mammalian physiological responses to a condensed tannin and its ecological implications. J. Mamm. , 72(3):480-486. Robbins, C.T., T.A. Hanley, A.E. Hagerman, O. Hjeljord, D.L. Baker, C.C. Schwartz, and W.W. Mautz 1987. Role of tannins in defending plants against ruminants: reduction in protein availability. Ecology, 68(1):98-107. 161 Rodgers, W.A. 1976. Seasonal diet preferences of impala from South East Tanzania. E. Afr. Wildl. J., 14:331-333. Root, RB. 1973. Organization of a plant-arthropod association in simple and diverse habitats: the fauna of collard (Brassica oleracea). Ecol. Monogr. , 43:95-120. Rosenthal, G.A. and EA. Bell 1979. Naturally occurring, toxic nonprotein amino acids. In Herbivores: Their Interaction with Secondary Plant Metabolites, pp. 353-385, eds. G.A. Rosenthal and D.H. Janzen. New York: Academic Press. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1979. A role for brown adipose tissue in diet-induced thermogenesis. Nature, 281:31-35. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1981a. Thermogenesis: comparative and evolutionary considerations. In The Body Weight Regulatory System: Normal and Disturbed Mechanisms, pp. 335-343, eds. L.A. Cioffi, W.P.T. James, and TB. Van Itallie. New York: Raven Press. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1981b. Rothwell and Stock reply. Nature, 289:699- 700. Rothwell, NJ. and M .J . Stock 1983. Luxuskonsumption, diet-induced thermogenesis and brown fat: the case in favor. Clin. Science, 64: 19-23. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1985a. Is diet-induced thermogenesis an experimental artefact? Br. J. Nutr., 53:191. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1985b. Biological distribution and significance of brown adipose tissue. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. , 82A(4):745-751. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1985c. Thermogenic capacity and brown adipose tissue activity in the common marmoset. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. , 81A(3):683-686. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1986a. Brown adipose tissue and diet-induced thermo- genesis. In Brown Adipose Tissue, pp. 269-298, eds. P. Trayhum and D.G. Nicholls. London: Edward Arnold. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1986b. Influence of environmental temperature on energy balance, diet-induced thermogenesis and brown fat activity in ‘cafete- ria’-fed rats. Br. J. Nutr., 56:123-129. Rothwell, NJ. and MJ . Stock 1987a. Diet-induced thermogenesis: concepts and mechanisms. J. Obes. and Wt. Reg., 6:147-161. 162 Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1987b. Effect of environmental temperature on energy balance and thermogenesis in rats fed normal or low protein diets. J. Nutr. , 117:833-837. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1987c. Influence of carbohydrate and fat intake on diet-induced thermogenesis and brown fat activity in rats fed low protein diets. J. Nutr., 117:1721-1726. Rothwell, NJ. and M.J. Stock 1988. The cafeteria diet as a tool for studies of thermogenesis. J. Nutr. , 118:925-928. Rothwell, NJ ., M.E. Saville, and M.J. Stock 1982. Effects of feeding a “cafeteria” diet on energy balance and diet-induced thermogenesis in four strains of rat. J. Nutr., 112:1515-1524. Rothwell, NJ ., M.J. Stock, and RS. Tyzbir 1982. Energy balance and mitochondrial function in liver and brown fat of rats fed “cafeteria” diets of varying protein content. J. Nutr., 112:1663-1672. Rothwell, NJ ., M.J. Stock, and RS. Tyzbir 1983a. Mechanisms of thermogenesis induced by low protein diets. Metabolism, 32(3):257-261. Rothwell, NJ ., M.J. Stock, and BF. Warwick 1983b. The effect of high fat and high carbohydrate cafeteria diets on diet-induced thermogenesis in the rat. Int. J. Obesity, 7:263-270. Rubner, M. 1902. Die Gesetze des Energieverbrauchs bei der Emahrung Deuticke, Leipzig. Scholander, RR, R. Hock, V. Walters, and L. Irving. 1950a. Adaptation to cold in arctic and tropical mammals and birds in relation to body temperature, insula- tion, and basal metabolic rate. Biol. Bull. , 99:259-271. Scholander, RR, R. Hock, V. Walters, F. Johnson, and L. Irving. 1950b. Heat regulation in some arctic and tropical mammals and birds. Biol. Bull. , 99:237- 258. Scholander, P.F., V. Walters, R. Hock, and L. Irving. 1950c. Body insulation of some arctic and tropical mammals and birds. Biol. Bull. , 99:225-236. Schultz, J .C., PJ. Nothnagle, and LT. Baldwin 1982. Seasonal and individual variation in leaf quality of two northern hardwoods tree species. Am. J. Bot., 69:753-759. 163 Schwartz, C.C. and N.T. Hobbs 1985. Forage and range evaluation. In Bioenergetics of Wild Herbivores, pp. 25-51, eds. RJ. Hudson and R.G. White. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Schwartz, C.C., W.L. Regelin, and LG. Nagy 1980. Deer preferences for juniper forage and volatile treated foods. J. Wildl. Manage, 44(1): 1 14-120. Scriber, J.M. 1977. Limiting effects of low leaf-water content on the nitrogen utilization, energy budget and larval growth of Hyalophora cecropia (Lepidop- tera: Satumiidae). Oecologia, 28:269-287. Scriber, J .M. 1984a. Host-plant suitability. In Chemical Ecology of Insects, pp. 160- 202, eds. W.J. Bell and RT. Carde. London: Chapman and Hall Ltd. Scriber, J .M. 1984b. Evolution of feeding specialization, physiological efficiency, and host races in selected Papilionidae and Satumiidae. In Variable Plants and Herbivores in Natural and Managed Systems, pp. 373-412, eds. R. F. Denno and M.S. McClure. New York: Academic Press. Scriber, J.M. and F. Slansky, Jr. 1981. The nutritional ecology of immature insects. Ann. Rev. Entomol., 26:183-211. Shenk, 1.8., RC. Elliot, and J .W. Thomas 1970. Meadow vole nutrition studies with semisynthetic diets. J. Nutr., 100:1437-1446. Shkolnik, A. and K. Schmidt-Nielsen 1976. Temperature regulation in hedgehogs from temperate and desert environments. Physiol. Zool. , 49:56-64. Sibly, RM. 1981. Strategies of digestion and defecation. In Physiological Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach to Resource Use, pp. 109-139, eds. C.R. Townsend and P. Calow. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Sinclair, A.R.E. 1974. The natural regulation of buffalo populations in East Africa. E. Afr. Wildl. J., 12:291-311. Sinclair, A.R.E. 1975. The resource limitation of trophic levels in tropical grassland ecosystems. J. Anim. Ecol., 44:497-520. Sinclair, A.R.E. 1979. The eruption of ruminants. In Serengeti: Dynamics of an Ecosystem, pp. 82-103, eds. A.R.E. Sinclair and M. Norton-Griffiths. Chica- go: University of Chicago Press. Slansky, F., Jr. and P. Feeny 1977. Stabilization of the rate of nitrogen accumulation by larvae of the cabbage butterfly on wild and cultivated food plants. Ecology, 47 :209-228. 164 Smith, J.N.M., CJ. Krebs, A.R.E. Sinclair, and R. Boonstra 1988. Population biology of snowshoe hares. II. Interactions with winter food plants. J. Anim. Ecol., 57:269-286. Spencer, K.C., ed., 1988. Chemical Mediation of Coevolution. San Diego: Academic Press. Stirling, LL. and M.J. Stock 1968. Metabolic origins of thermogenesis induced by diet. Nature, 220:801-802. Swain, T. 1979. Tannins and lignins. In Herbivores: Their Interaction with Second- ary Plant Metabolites, pp. 657-682, eds. G.A. Rosenthal and D.H. Janzen. New York: Academic Press. Swick, R.W. and NJ. Benevenga 1977. labile protein reserves and protein turnover. J. Dairy Science, 60(4):505-515. Swick, R.W. and CL. Gribskov 1983. The effect of dietary protein levels on diet- induced thermogenesis in the rat. J. Nutr. , 113:2289-2294. Swick, R.W. and M.F. Henningfield 1989. Changes in the number of GDP binding sites on brown adipose tissue (BAT) mitochondria and its uncoupling protein. In Hormones, Thermogenesis, and Obesity, pp. 117-127, eds. H. Lardy and F. Stratman. New York: Elsevier. Swick, A.G. and R.W. Swick 1986. Enhanced thermogenic response to a diet low but adequate in protein persists in older rats. Am. J. Physiol. , 251:FA38-E441. Tabashnik, BE. and ES. Slansky, Jr. 1987. Nutritional ecology of forb foliage- chewing insects. In Nutritional Ecology of Insects, Mites, Spiders, and Related Invertebrates, pp. 71-103, eds. F. Slansky, Jr. and J .G. Rodriquez. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Tahvanainen, J ., E. Helle, R. J ulkunen-Titto, and A. Lavola 1985. Phenolic com- pounds of willow bark as deterrents against feeding by mountain hare. Oeco- logia, 65:319-323. Tahvanainen, J .O. and RB. Root 1972. The influence of vegetational diversity on the populational ecology of a specialized herbivore, Phyllotreta cruciferae (Cole- optera: Chrysomelidae). Oecologia, 10:321-346. Taitt, M.J. and CJ. Krebs 1981. The effect of extra food on small rodent popula- tions. 11. Voles (Microtus townsendir). J. Anim. Ecol.50:125-137. 165 Taitt, M.J. and Q]. Krebs 1983. Predation, cover, and food manipulations during a spring decline in Microtus townsendii. J. Anim. Ecol. , 52:837-848. Thomas, D.W. 1984. Fruit intake and energy budgets of frugivorous bats. Physiol. Zool., 57(4):457-467. Thomas, D.W., C. Samson, and J .M. Bergeron 1988. Metabolic costs associated with the ingestion of plant phenolics by Microtus pennsylvanicus. J. Mamm. , 69(3):512-515. Tobin, G., G. Armitage, and G.R. Hervey 1981. Energy expenditure during cafeteria feeding in rats. Int. J. Obesity, 5:379-384. Trayhum, P. and R.E. Milner. 1987. Mechanisms of thermogenesis: brown adipose tissue. J. Obesity and Wt. Reg., 6:147-161. Tulp, O.L., P.P. Krupp, E. Danforth, Jr. and ES. Horton 1979. Charactristics of thyroid function in experimental protein malnutrition. J. Nutr., 109:1321-1332. Uresk, D.W. and P.L. Sims 1975 . Influence of grazing on crude protein content of blue grama. J. Range Mgrnt., 28:370—37 1. Van Alstyne, K.L. 1988. Herbivore grazing increases polyphenolic defenses in the intertidal brown alga Fucus distichus. Ecology, 69(3):655-663. Van Hoven, W. and E. A. Boomker 1985. Digestion. In Bioenergetics of Wild Herbivores, pp. 103-120, eds. RJ. Hudson and R.G. White. Boca Raton: CRC Press. Van Soest, PJ. 1968. Structural and chemical characteristics which limit the nutritive value of forages. In Forage Economics—Quality, pp. 63-76, eds. C.M. Harrison, M. Stelly, and S.A. Breth. Madison: American Society of Agrono- my. Van Soest, PJ. 1982. Nutritional Ecology of the Ruminant. Corvallis: O & B Books. Vander Tuig, J .G., J. Kemer, and DR. Romsos 1985. Hypothalamic obesity, brown adipose tissue, and sympathoadrenal activity in rats. Am. J. Physiol. , 248(11):E607-E617. Vander Tuig, J .G. and DR. Romsos 1984. Effects of dietary carbohydrate, fat, and protein on norepinephrine turnover in rats. Metabolism, 33(1):26-33. 166 Vaughan, M.R. and L.B. Keith 1981. Demographic response of experimental snow- shoe hare populations to overwinter food shortage. J. Wildl. Manage. , 45 :354- 380. Vaughan, T.A. and NJ. Czaplewski 1985. Reproduction in Stephens’ woodrat: the wages of folivery. J. Mamm., 66:429-443. Vickery, W.L. 1984. Optimal diet models and rodent food consumption. Anim. Behav. , 32:340-348. Voit, E. 1901. Uber die Grosse des Energiebedarfs der Tiere in Hungerzustande. Ztschr. Biol. 41:113-154. Waldbauer, GP. 1968. The consumption and utilization of food by insects. In Advances in Insect Physiology, pp. 229-288, eds. J.W.L. Beament, J.B. Treherne, and VB. Wigglesworth. London: Academic Press. Waterlow, J .C. 1985. What do we mean by adaptation? In Nutritional Adaptation in Man, pp. l-11, eds. K. Blaxter and LC. Waterlow. London: John Libbey. Watt, AD. 1987. The effect of shoot growth stage of Pinus contorta and Pinus sylvestris on the growth and survival of Panolis flammea larvae. Oecologia, 72:429-433. Watt, AD. 1989. The chemical composition of pine foliage in relation to the popula- tion dynamics of the pine beauty moth, Panolis flammea, in Scotland. Oeco- logia, 78:251-258. Webster, AJ .F. 1981. The energetic efficiency of metabolism. Proc. Nutr. Soc. , 40:121-128. Webster, A.J.F. 1983. Energetics of maintenance and growth. In Mammalian Ther- mogenesis, pp. 178-207, eds. L. Girardier and M.J. Stock. London: Chapman and Hall. White, R. G. 1983. Foraging patterns and their multiplier effects on productivity of northern ungulates. Oikos 40:377-384. White, T.C.R. 1978. The importance of a relative shortage of food in animal ecolo- gy. Oecologia, 33:71—86. Wilkinson, L. 1989. SYSTAT: The System for Statistics for the PC. Evanston: SYSTAT, Inc. 167 Windberg, LA. and L.B. Keith 1976. Snowshoe hare population response to artificial high densities. J. Mamm., 57:523-553. Wunder, BA. 1985 . Energetics and thermoregualtion. In Biology of New World Microtus, pp. 812-844, ed. R.H. Tamarin. Am. Soc. Mamm. Spec. Pub. No. 8. Wunder, B.A., D.S. Dobkin, and RD. Gettinger 1977. Shifts of thermogenesis in the prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster). Oecologia, 29:11-26. Young, J.B., L.N. Kaufman, M.E. Saville, and L. Landsberg 1985. Increased sympathetic nervous system activity in rats fed a low-protein diet. Am. J. Physiol. , 248(17):R627-R637. Young, J.B., E. Saville, NJ. Rothwell, M.J. Stock, and L. Landsberg 1982. Effect of diet and cold exposure on norepinephrine turnover in brown adipose tissue of the rat. J. Clin. Invest., 69:1061-1071. Young, T.P. 1987. Increased thorn length in Acacia depranolobium—an induced response to browsing. Oecologia, 71(3):436-438. Zangerl, A.R. and M.R. Berenbaum 1990. Furanocoumarin induction in wild parsnip: genetics and populational variation. Ecology, 71(5): 1933-1940. Zhang, X. and LS. States 1991. Selective herbivory of ponderosa pine by Abert squirrels: a re-examination of the role of terpenes. Biochem. Syst. Ecol. 19(2):111-115. ”llllllllllllllf