lllllllllllllll’l‘llllllll illllllilll 3 1293 01050 0019 LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled A CRITIQUE OF JOHN OGBU'S MINORITY TYPOLOGY presented by Shelley'L- Brown has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. degree inSoQiologL AMQM" Z 59“ a Major professor Date Jam—1.044336. 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution me: u RETURN sex to roman this chockoui from your mom. TO AVOID FINES mum on or Moro dd. duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU loAn Minn-tin Action/Equal Opportunity Ira-titular. W m1 A CRITIQUE or JOHN OGBU’S MINORITY TYPOLOGY By Shelley L. Brown A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Sociology 1996 ABSTRACT A CRITIQUE OF JOHN OGBU’S MINORITY TYPOLOGY By Shelley L. Brown Although credited for providing a more in-depth understanding of the historical circumstances and complex processes affecting minority student performance and immigrant adaption, John Ogbu’s minority typology is criticized for underestimating the diversifying effect of class on the lives of people of color in the United States. Using data from the National Educational Longitudinal Study, this paper attempts to expand Ogbu’s theory by examining the role that socioeconomic class plays in shaping aspirations, influencing success strategies, and predicting academic outcomes among minority populations. However, findings suggest that, although positive relationships do exist between socioeconomic class and each of these three variables, Ogbu’s minority typology remains a better predictor. To my mother and father, Albert and Delores Brown, for their unconditional love and support. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .............................................................................................. vi LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................... vii INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................. 1 CHAPTER 1 OGBU’S PERSPECTIVE .................................................................................... 2 The Role of Institutionalized Discrimination .......................................... 2 Immigrant Minorities .............................................................................. 4 Involuntary Minorities ............................................................................. 5 Adaptive Responses .................................................................................. 6 CHAPTER 2 THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVE ....................................................................... 9 Cultural Deficiency .................................................................................. 9 Structural Theories ................................................................................. 10 Expanding the Idea of Innovation ......................................................... 14 Applying the Theories ............................................................................ 15 CHAPTER 3 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM ................................................................ 17 CHAPTER 4 METHOD OF DATA COLLECTION ............................................................. 21 CHAPTER 5 OPERATIONALIZATION OF TERMS ........................................................... 22 Socioeconomic Class .............................................................................. 22 Self-Efficacy ............................................................................................ 23 iv Constructive and Deconstructive Innovation ........................................ 23 Academic Outcomes ............................................................................... 24 CHAPTER 6 ANALYSIS OF DATA ...................................................................................... 26 CHAPTER 7 CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS .......................................... 31 APPENDICES .................................................................................................. 35 BIBLIOGRAPHY .............................................................................................. 42 LIST OF TABLES Table 1 - Correlation Matrix: Socioeconomic Class and Self-Efficacy ............. 38 Table 2 - Correlation Matrix: Socioeconomic Class and Innovative Response Type ................................................................................................... 39 Table 3 - Correlation Matrix: Socioeconomic Class and Academic Outcome. 40 LIST OF FIGURES Figure l - Ogbu’s Theoretical Model .................................................................. 3 Figure 2 - Merton’s Theory of Structural Strain .............................................. 11 Figure 3 - Proposed Theoretical Model ............................................................ 20 INTRODUCTION Education is a valuable asset in today’s high-tech, global economy. It plays a crucial role in offering opportunities for individual mobility while at the same time legitimating structural inequalities. Consequently, it is important to develop and maintain strategies that allow all students to maximize their educational potential and to identify social and economic circumstances that perpetuate existing disparities by encouraging academic success or failure. If armed with such information, educators and policy makers might be better able to understand and, as a result, better able to address the needs of the many diverse populations that they serve. Emphasizing structural as opposed to cultural explanations for significant differences in the academic performance of immigrant versus involuntary minority groups, this study attempts to provide a more in-depth understanding by comparing the effect of mode of incorporation and economic class on aspirations, success strategies, and educational achievement. Chapter 1 OGBU’S PERSPECTIVE The Role of Institutionalized Discrimination: Ogbu faults structural inequality in both the economic and educational systems for contributing to many problems that minority students face in school (see Figure 1 on the following page). According to Ogbu (19785), there is a reciprocal relationship between the opportunities open to a group in American society and the pattern of linguistic, cognitive, motivational, and other school-related skills they develop. By not allowing minorities entry into the labor force and advancement according to their educational qualifications and ability and by denying them adequate rewards for their education in terms of wages, American society discourages minority youth from investing time and effort into the pursuit of education and maximization of their educational accomplishments (Ogbu, 1987:318). Because schools operate according to the norms of American society and according to the norms of the communities in which they exist, Ogbu (1987:319) suggests that they also contribute to the academic problems of minority children, intentionally or unintentionally, by denigrating minorities and their culture. 2 Figure l Ogbu’s Theoretical Model Source: Ogbu, 1983 4 However, Gibson and Ogbu (1991 :8) claim that there are two historical forces which shape the different cultural models of minority groups who are relatively successful or unsuccessful in school. One is the group’s initial terms of incorporation into the society in which they presently exist; and the other is the pattern of adaptive responses that the group has made subsequent to discriminatory treatment inflicted by members of the dominant group. Immigrant Minorities: Immigrant minorities are groups who move to the United States more or less voluntarily in search of increased economic opportunity, better overall well-being, and/or greater political freedom. Within the United States, the Chinese, Cubans, Filipinos, Japanese, and Koreans constitute immigrant minorities (Ogbu, 1978, 1983, and 1987). Because they retain the sense of peoplehood they had prior to emigration, immigrant minorities are characterized by what Ogbu (1978 and 1987) calls “primary cultural systems”. Comparing their current conditions of existence in the host society with the standard endured by peers “back home”, immigrant minorities develop favorable perceptions of the opportunity structure in the United States, viewing discrimination as a temporary response to their “guest” status, linguistic accents, or lack of an American education. Consequently, immigrant minorities view education as the avenue to economic success and upward mobility. Involuntary Minorities: Involuntary minorities are groups brought into their present society through slavery, conquest, or colonization. Within the United States, African Americans, Native Americans, Hawaiians, and Alaskans, Puerto Ricans, and Mexican Americans constitute involuntary minorities (Ogbu, 1978, 1983, and 1987). Because their original cultural identity is destroyed and/or degraded during the incorporation process, Ogbu (1978, 1983, and 1987) says that involuntary minorities are characterized by “secondary cultural systems” under which they create a new cultural identity in response to the unjust treatment inflicted upon them by members of dominant group. Through historical experience, involuntary groups learn that discrimination is permanent and institutionalized. By comparing their status with that of members of the dominant group, involuntary minorities also realize that the rules for advancement do not work as well for them as they do for members of the dominant group. Involuntary minorities discover that it requires more than education, individual effort, and hard work to overcome the barriers against them in the opportunity structure. Adaptive Responses: Unlike involuntary minorities, immigrant minorities do not view cultural differences between themselves and the dominant group as markers of identity to be maintained. Instead, Ogbu (1987) and Gibson and Ogbu (1991) find that within immigrant communities cultural differences are seen as barriers to be overcome in order to achieve long range goals of future employment. Because they employ an “additive” method of cultural adaption, immigrant minorities do not interpret the incorporation of dominant cultural features as threatening to their own culture, language, or identity but as a necessary requirement for social adjustment and successful academic performance. Through gossip and related techniques, immigrant parents and communities promote good work habits and perseverance, communicating to children nonambivalent, instrumental messages about education. Consequently, immigrant children develop and maintain serious academic attitudes, value making good grades, follow school rules and standard practices, and invest a good deal of time and effort in their schoolwork They often select courses requiring less use of language and avoid fields of study that prepare them for jobs where there is a job ceiling or discrimination against their group. 7 Involuntary minorities, on the other hand, practice “cultural inversion” as an adaptive response to oppression (Ogbu, 1978, 1983, and 1987; Gibson and Ogbu, 1991). Through the process of cultural inversion, involuntary minorities define certain forms of behavior, events, symbols, and meanings as inappropriate for them, because they are identified with members of the dominant group and regard other, often oppositional, forms of behavior, events, symbols, and meanings as appropriate for them, because they are not associated with the dominant group. As a result of this oppositional or “subtractive” method of cultural adaption, involuntary minorities do not interpret the acquisition of academic learning and skills associated with the dominant culture as an additional set of skills to be drawn upon when appropriate but rather as an indication of subservience. Among the adaptive strategies employed by involuntary minority parents and communities are passive confrontation with the schools; verbal encouragement but nonteaching involvement with children’s education; a weak control of children’s use of time; a weak socialization of children to develop good academic work habits and perseverance at academic tasks; and a weak sanction of academic instrumental behavior and academic responsibility. As a consequence, involuntary minority youth do not develop or maintain good academic work habits and attitudes. Instead, they tend to have a norm of 8 minimum effort, spend limited time on academic tasks, and avoid taking difficult or “White” courses. According to Ogbu (1987), involuntary minority youth tend to be satisfied with average grades and submit easily to peer pressures that take them away from their schoolwork. They resist following school rules and standard practices and distrust school authorities with whom they are frequently in conflict. Plagued by truancy, delinquency, and high drop-out rates, involuntary minority youth often change the rules for economic advancement (Ogbu, 1978). Having rejected the educational expectations of both the schools and the society, many young people in this group direct their time and energy into nonacademic, less socially acceptable areas as a means of getting ahead. The street economy is one popular alternative. Chapter 2 THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVE Ogbu’s minority typology is based upon two general theoretical models, the first focuses upon cultural deficiency, and the second centers on structural opportunity. Cultural Deficiency: Like many deficiency theorists (see Blauner, 1972; Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck, 1961; Moynihan, 1965; Solomon, 1988; Sowell, 1981), Ogbu’s explanations for racial subordination often focus on pathological cultural characteristics thought to be inherited from the past and reproduced from one generation to the next. Because cultural differences between immigrant and involuntary minorities are treated as cultural deficiencies and assumed to be the reason that involuntary minorities are disproportionately found in subordinate positions, Ogbu and others sharing this perspective seem to criticize less successful groups for not modeling the behavior of immigrant minorities. Arguing that the attitudes and values held by members of 10 involuntary minority groups often prevent them from successfully competing in society, cultural deficiency theorists claim that involuntary minority communities adopt cultural identities that are characterized by complex webs of deviant behavioral patterns. Consequently, involuntary minorities are judged to be more permissive in raising their children, less verbal, more fatalistic, less apt to defer gratification, and less likely to be interested in formal education than members of immigrant minority groups. Unlike many deficiency theorists, Ogbu points out that many of these practices are adaptations to oppressive structural conditions. Structural Theories: Arguing that racism is an institutionalized component of all major sectors of society, structural theorists (see Carrier, 1986; Cloward, 1968; Oakes, 1985; Persell and Cookson, 1990; Woodson, 1992) claim that the criminal justice, economic, health care, and/or formal educational systems interact to regulate members of minority groups to subordinate positions within the social and economic power structure where the opportunities left open to them are drastically limited. Focusing on the response patterns adopted by minority group members in reaction to limited opportunity, the theory on which Ogbu’s minority typology is most closely based is Merton’s (1968) theory of structural strain which is summarized in Figure 2. ll imam Got News Reteaflsm — '- Ritualism - + Rebellion + l— +/— Comm + + Innovation + — Figure l Merton’s Theory of Structural Strain 12 Merton’s theory of structural strain is based upon three factors: (1) culturally valued goals defined as legitimate for all members of society, (2) norms that specify the legitimate means of achieving these goals, and (3) the actual number of legitimate opportunities available to people to achieve the culturally valued goals. According to Merton, structural strain occurs when the valued goals have no clear boundaries; when people are unsure whether the legitimate means that society provides will lead to the valued goals; or when legitimate opportunities for meeting the goals are closed to a significant portion of the population. Because Ogbu (1978, 1983, and 1987) states that they are often denied access to quality education, which is the socially accepted means of achieving the goal of upward mobility, and because their level of occupational advancement and financial reward is unjustly limited, even in cases where a quality education has been obtained, minority groups suffer structural strain. Although it is the individual’s job to choose a path that leads to success, Merton claims that people respond to structural strain in five identifiable ways, each involving some combination of acceptance and rejection of the valued goals and means. First, retreatism involves the rejection of both cultural goals and the means of achieving those goals. Unable to achieve success through 13 either legitimate or illegitimate means, retreatists resign from society. The retreatist not only rejects education as a means to advancement, but he or she abandons all nonacademic avenues as well. The second response, ritualism, involves the rejection of cultural goals but rigid adherence to the legitimate means of achieving those goals. Although the ritualist plays by society’s rules, he or she does not expect hard work to pay off in the form of financial success. Passively accepting his or her ascribed social status, the ritualist does not seriously pursue the goal of upward mobility. Rebellion, the third response, involves the full or partial rejection of both goals and means and the introduction of a new set of goals and means. According to Merton, rebellion provides for the potential formation of deviant subgroups, such as delinquent youth gangs. The fourth response, conformity, entails the acceptance of the cultural goals and the pursuit of those goals through legitimate means. Assuming that if they follow the rules they will be justly rewarded, conforrnists do not acknowledge the potential effect of economic inequality. Finally, innovation involves the acceptance of the cultural goals but rejection of the legitimate means to obtaining those goals. An innovator desires upward mobility but does not necessarily view education as the means 14 to achieving success. Expanding the Idea of Innovation: Although Merton’s (1968) description of innovation involves complete rejection of the culturally valued means of achieving success, Ogbu (1987), Gibson (1988 and 1995), and Gibson and Ogbu (1991) point out that innovation does not necessarily entail total rejection of the legitimate means. There are both constructive and deconstructive forms of innovation. Recognizing the additional barriers that they must overcome in order to achieve culturally valued goals, deconstructive innovators continue to pursue economic success and upward mobility but choose unproductive, even destructive, methods of countering these obstacles. Unintentionally exacerbating existing problems and inequalities, deconstructive innovators often reject positive aspects of the dominant culture that may have been beneficial to members of their group. Because it discourages serious scholarly effort and respect for school authority, the subtractive or oppositional mode of cultural adaption employed by involuntary minorities is considered a deconstructive form of innovation. However, the additive method of innovation practiced by immigrant minorities does encourage academic excellence and is, therefore, considered a constructive form of innovation. After evaluating all possible alternatives, constructive innovators, unlike 15 deconstructive innovators, choose more productive, socially acceptable methods of countering the effects of institutionalized discrimination in their pursuit of culturally valued goals. Selectively incorporating positive aspects of the dominant culture that may be beneficial to members of their group, constructive innovators encourage “accommodation without assimilation”. Consequently, education remains instrumental in their pursuit of economic success and upward mobility. Applying the Theories: Despite their high drop out rates which imply that involuntary minorities, like retreatists, reject education as a means of achieving upward mobility, and the lack of motivation they hold in common with ritualists, Ogbu (1978 and 1983) suggests that, unlike either retreatists or ritualists, most involuntary minorities do accept the cultural goal of social and economic advancement, although many attempt to achieve it through socially unacceptable, nonacademic avenues. Consequently, very few from either minority group may be accurately classified as retreatists or ritualists. As for more rebellious forms of reaction, Merton (1968) claims that such extreme patterns of response are confined only to small portions of the population. Although Ogbu (1978 and 1983) suggests that involuntary groups exhibit the greatest potential for rebellion, especially disorganized forms such 16 as riots, this response is least frequently used. Finally, adhering strictly to the rules and underestimating the pervasiveness of racial discrimination, Ogbu’s description of immigrant minorities suggests that they tend to be conforrnists. However, Boddy (1970), Fong (1994), Miyamoto (1984), and Takagi (1992) document both subtle and blatant forms of racial discrimination that have historically plagued immigrant communities and continue, presently, to limit their life chances. According to Omi and Winant (1991) and Min (1996), common experiences of racial oppression, such as these, tend to intensify group identity and cohesion. Consequently, for many immigrant minorities who choose to remain in the US. over an extended period of time conformity may be only a temporary response decreasing in appeal as members of immigrant minority groups become more “racialized”. Because most accept the cultural goals of upward mobility and economic success but disagree as to the most appropriate means of achieving these common goals, the great majority of involuntary and immigrant minority youth tend to be innovators, with the former group being more apt to employ deconstructive forms of innovation and the latter group gravitating toward more constructive forms of innovation. For this reason, neither retreatism, ritualism, rebellion, nor conformity will be further discussed in this study. Chapter 3 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM One of the reasons cited by Ogbu (1978:8) for his conscious omission of class within the African-American community is his belief that, “Regardless of their class position, Blacks tend to perceive of their opportunities for social mobility as much more limited than is the case for the general population.” Because Ogbu fails to consider class variables among any of the other involuntary minority groups, this belief appears to be generalizable. In addition, Ogbu also assumes that, regardless of their class position, immigrant minorities hold equally favorable perceptions of the U. S. opportunity structure and are equally optimistic about their ability to achieve upward mobility through legitimate means. However, as Gibson and Ogbu (1991:371) admit, The cultural models and educational strategies of minority communities are in a constant process of renegotiation. Mobility strategies change as the societal context changes and as the minority group’s situation within a given society itself changes. Both role expectations and folk theories of success are modified over time. 17 18 Although US. society continues to be plagued by structural inequality and unequal power relations, the occupations above the job ceiling of which Ogbu (1978) speaks include “clerical, sales, and kindred workers, professionals, technicians, managers, officials, proprietors, and skilled craftsmen and foremen”; and involuntary group members have, recently, made considerable advances in many of these areas, thereby penetrating this job ceiling. Has the rise of the middle class in Black and other involuntary minority communities served to diversify theories, beliefs, and experiences (Frazier, 1962; Fanon, 1967; Benjamin, 1991)? Because of the many motivational factors that fuel migration, Pedraza (1996), Rumbaut (1996), and Portes and Rumbaut (1990), also, note significant differences in socioeconomic class among laborers, professionals, entrepreneurs, refugees, and asylees within immigrant communities. Because migration is often characterized by an inverse relationship between date of departure and social class of the immigrants, particularly among refugees, class disparities within immigrant communities may widen in the future. Will these increases in class disparity influence the role expectations and mobility strategies employed by immigrant minorities? If so, in what ways? Ogbu fails to address questions such as these. According to findings introduced by Deosaran (1978); Evans (1990 and 19 1993); Gecas and Schwalbe (1983); Gecas, Schwalbe, and Staples (1984) and Hughes and Demo (1989), a positive relationship exists between socioeconomic class and self-efficacy (or one’s self-perceived ability to achieve intended goals through legitimate means). If this finding holds true, then minority students from more privileged class backgrounds should express higher degrees of self-efficacy. Because students with higher degrees of self- efficacy are, by definition, more certain of their ability to achieve intended goals through legitimate, socially acceptable means, minorities from more privileged class backgrounds should, also, be more willing to accept education as the means to obtaining economic success. Consequently, minorities from privileged class backgrounds should be more inclined to adopt constructive forms of innovation which produce higher academic outcomes, irrespective of their group’s mode of incorporation. On the other hand, minority youth from less privileged class backgrounds should experience lower degrees of self- efficacy. Believing that they will not be able to succeed through legitimate means, minorities from less privileged class backgrounds will be more likely to adopt deconstructive forms of innovation which tend to result in lower academic outcomes, regardless of their group’s method of entry. These predictions are based upon three major assumptions which are illustrated in Figure 3: 20 Assumptionfln: There are positive relationships between socioeconomic class and (a) self-efficacy; (b) innovative response type, with constructive innovation being the more highly rated type of response; and (c) academic outcome. When socioeconomic class is held constant, significant proportions of variance in academic outcome are explained independently by (a) self-efficacy and (b) innovative response type. W When socioeconomic class is held constant, the amount of variance in academic outcome explained by a minority group’s mode of incorporation decreases. HI Hit “m“ I... ca: HI lHl WI+ l ”I l Law low Low Figure 3 Proposed Theoretical Model Chapter 4 METHOD OF DATA COLLECTION Data for this study was taken from the National Educational Longitudinal Study (NELS). Beginning in 1988 with a cohort of 25,000 eighth graders attending 1,000 public and private schools across the nation, the NELS was designed to provide data about critical transitions experienced by students as they leave eighth grade school settings, progress through high school (or dropout), enter and leave postsecondary institutions, and enter the work force. The 1988 eighth grade cohort has been followed at two-year intervals, and the data for this study was collected during the second follow-up which took place in 1992, when most sample members were entering the second semester of their senior year. Because this study focuses on minority students, all cases listed as “non-Hispanic White” have been eliminated from consideration leaving a total sample size of 7,164 cases including 4,626 involuntary minorities, 1,214 immigrant minorities, and 1,324 minorities of unknown origin. 21 Chapter 5 OPERATIONALIZATION OF TERMS Socioeconomic Class: In order to evaluate the influence of socioeconomic class background on the response patterns of students from both minority groups two indicators, f2pared and f25eslq, are used. F2pared measures the educational level of the respondent’s parent(s) , and f23es 1 q indicates the respondent’s socioeconomic quartile. F2 ses l q is a string variable constructed by dividing another variable, f2sesl , into four quartiles based on the weighted marginal distribution. F25esl , a continuous variable with a mean of 16.89 and standard deviation of 37.87, was created using base year parent questionnaire data. When available the following parent data were used: father’s education level, mother’s education level, father’s occupation, mother’s occupation, and family income. Finally, occupational data were recoded using the Duncan’s Socioeconomic Index. 22 23 Self-Efficacy: The data set contains three items, f2567a, f2$67b, and f2s43, which indicate the respondent’s self-perceived chances of succeeding through legitimate, socially acceptable means. F2367a indicates the respondent’s chances of graduating from high school, f2$67b indicates his or her chances of graduating from college, and f2$43 reflects how far in school the respondent thinks that he or she will get. Positive relationships between socioeconomic class variables (f2pared and f25eslq) and each of the three indicators of self- efficacy (f2s43, f2s67b, and f2$67a) confirm assumption 1(a). Constructive and Deconstructive Innovation: Constructive and deconstructive forms of innovation are measured by five item indicators including f259b, f2$9d, f2s25f2, f2$34a, and hsprgrn. Item f2$9b indicates how often the respondent cuts classes; item f2$9d indicates how often he or she gets into trouble for breaking school rules; item f2s25f2 indicates the amount of time that the respondent spends on homework; item f2s34a indicates the amount of time that he or she spends playing video games; and item hsprgrn indicates whether the respondent is enrolled in a college preparatory program. Because constructive and deconstructive innovation are two sides to the same coin, high scores for items indicating one type of innovation translate into low scores for the same items indicating the 24 other. In this case, constructive innovation is indicated by high scores for items f2$25f2 and hsprgm and low scores for items f2$9b, f2s9d, and f2$34a. This response pattern is reversed in deconstructive innovators. Positive relationships between socioeconomic class variables (f2pared and f2$eslq) and constructive forms of innovation (f2325f2 and hsprgm), in addition to negative relationships between socioeconomic class variables (f2pared and f2seslq) and deconstructive types of innovation (f239b, f259d, and f2334a) confirm assumption 1(b). Academic Outcomes: The three items indicating academic outcome are grade point average (gpa), notdrop, and f22xcomp. Gpa is a continuous variable with a mean of 2.50 and a standard deviation of .89. Although it is based upon a 4.00 scale, gpa consists of scores ranging in value from 0.00 to 5 .00, with scores above 4.00 indicating extra credit granted to respondents enrolled in honors or exceptionally difficult courses. Notdrop indicates whether or not the respondent continues to be enrolled in school. Finally, f22xcomp is a continuous variable with a mean of 47.30 and standard deviation of 10.22. These values represent standardized test composites combining cognitive test scores achieved in mathematics and reading comprehension. The math test on which this variable is partially based contained items such as word problems, 25 graphs, equations, quantitative comparisons, and geometric figures. Some questions could be answered by simple application of skills or knowledge, others required the student to demonstrate a more advanced level of comprehension and problem solving. The test of reading comprehension contained four to five short reading passages, with three to five questions about the content of each. Questions encompassed understanding the meaning of words in context, identifying figures of speech, interpreting the author’s perspective, and evaluating the passage as a whole. Positive relationships between variables indicating socioeconomic class (f2pared and f2ses 1 q) and items (gpa, f22xcomp, and notdrop) indicating academic outcome confirm assumption 1(c). See Appendix A for further clarification. Chapter 6 ANALYSIS OF DATA Data in this study is analyzed using frequencies, correlation matrices, comparison of means, and multiple regression. Measures of association include Kendaull’s Tau-b, eta, and chi square, and all relationships are significant below a probability level of .05. Assumptignflne There are positive relationships between socioeconomic class and (a) self-efficacy; (b) innovative response type, with constructive innovation being the more highly rated type of response; and (c) academic outcome. Although data in Table 1 (see Appendix B) suggest that all six relationships are weak---the strongest, 11, = .298, between f23eslq and f2$67b and the weakest, Tb = .045, between f23eslq and f2543-«assumption 1(a) is confirmed by positive correlations between socioeconomic class variables and each of the three indicators of self-efficacy. All correlations including indicators of self-efficacy are significant below p = .001. As predicted by assumption 1(b) and indicated by data in Table 2 (see 26 27 Appendix C), significant positive correlations between socioeconomic class variables and those indicating constructive forms of innovation do exist, although most are very weak Because constructive and deconstructive innovation are two sides to the same coin, in order to confirm this assumption completely, negative correlations should exist between socioeconomic class variables and those indicating deconstructive forms of innovation. However, Table 2 indicates that although all specified correlations are negative, as predicted, f2534a is the only indicator of deconstructive innovation that is significantly related to both socioeconomic class variables at probability levels less than .001. While f259d is significantly associated only with f2pared at a probability level less than .05. Finally, data presented in Table 3 (see Appendix D) indicate that correlations between socioeconomic class variables and indicators of academic outcome appear to be stronger than correlations between socioeconomic class variables and those in either of the other two categories. Assumption 1(c) is confirmed by the six positive correlations in Table 3 which range in value from Tb = .215, between f25eslq and notdrop, to Tb = .440, between f25eslq and f22xcomp. In this instance all relationships are significant below p = .001. When socioeconomic class is held constant, significant proportions of variance in academic outcome are explained independently by (a) self-efficacy and (b) innovative response 28 type. Because the first regression equation (see Appendix E) indicates that f2$43, f2567a, and f2$67b (all indicators of self-efficacy) and f2$9b, f2$9d, f2s25f2, f2$34a, and hsprgm (all indicators of innovative response type) each explain very low, although significant, proportions of variance in gpa while controlling for socioeconomic class, both sections of assumption two are confirmed regarding this particular indicator of academic outcome. However, regression equations explaining the other two indicators of academic outcome offer less support. In the regression equation where y = f22xcomp, one indicator of self- efficacy (f2343) and two indicators of innovative response type (f239b and f259d) drop out of the equation. Only f2$67a, f2$67b, f2325f2, and f2$34a continue to explain significant proportions of variance in f22xcomp when socioeconomic class is held constant. Nevertheless, when combined these few variables provide greater predictive power than do those listed in the first equation, even though a greater number of variables are included in the former. Based upon the value of R square, the second equation explains 5.86 percent more variance in f22xcomp than the first equation explains in gpa. In addition, T-values corresponding to variables in the regression equation where y = f22xcomp are all significant below p = .001 , while none of the variables in 29 the equation where y = gpa have T-values that remain significant at such a low level of probability. The regression equation offering the least support for assumption two, however, is the one in which y = notdrop. In this final equation, all indicators of innovative response type and all but one indicator of self-efficacy fail to explain significant proportions of variance in notdrop when socioeconomic class is held constant Only one variable, f2367a, remains in the equation. Explaining only 1.04 percent of the variance in notdrop, the predictive power of this equation is very weak Wance in academic outcome explained by a minority group’s mode of incorporation decreases when socioeconomic class is held constant. Because all variables indicating socioeconomic class and mode of incorporation are excluded from the third regression equation, neither class factors nor method of entry appear to be significant predictors of a minority student’s decision to withdraw from or remain in school. Consequently, comparisons between the two were impossible in this case. Based upon data presented in the first two regression equations which suggest that academic outcome is positively influenced by immigrant status while involuntary status has the opposite effect, a minority group’s mode of incorporation explains more variance in both gpa and f22xcomp than all but 30 one other variable in both equations, even when controlling for socioeconomic class. Although socioeconomic class variables, especially f2pared, are significant predictors of gpa and f22xcomp when mode of incorporation is held constant, socioeconomic class factors fail to explain much of the variance in these forms of academic outcome among minority group members who have entered the US. voluntarily and those upon whom entry into the US. has been forced Therefore, assumption three remains unconfirmed. Chapter 7 CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS These findings suggest that, although several variables in the proposed theoretical model significantly explain small proportions of variance in academic outcome, especially gpa and composite test scores, there is something about being an immigrant as opposed to an involuntary minority that even outweighs the influence ‘of increasing economic opportunity in explaining differences in academic success. There are many possible explanations for this trend. Often possessing both social and cultural capital at the time of arrival, immigrant minorities have benefited from a greater number of resources. Unlike involuntary populations which represent a random selection, immigrant minorities are self-selected for the success struggle before ever reaching these shores. This distinction alone, may account for differences in level of motivation. In addition, immigration laws’ preferences for families, after years of denying families, helped establish network and support communities which 31 32 promoted the persistence of primary cultural systems along with any psychological comforts that these systems may provide. Preference to those with skills made finding jobs easier, and a shift in immigration from rural to urban immigrants who were more educated and to a greater degree professional, changed the trajectory of this population as a whole. Despite little economic capital, their trade or entrepreneurial skills, the social capital of existing networks available from earlier immigrants, and the small size of many immigrant populations enabled them to take advantage of market niches and assume “middleman minority” positions. Involuntary minorities can be seen as “immigrants” only arriving to the United States upon being freed from slavery or having gained independence from colonial rule and exploitation, at which point they faced economic and structural conditions unlike those encountered by more recent immigrant minority groups. Because factors such as basis for group conflict, resources upon arrival, structure of the economy at arrival, and size of the group interact, relegating minority groups to unequal positions within the “racial caste system,” involuntary minorities were often placed at a greater disadvantage from the very start. Because American society was historically organized around racist principles, racial harmony depends upon an elaborate system of controls to insure compliance. Immigrant groups are less threatening, 33 therefore, the need to control involuntary minorities has been greater for the dominant group. Deprived of individual freedoms and denied education during the colonial period, involuntary minorities had neither economic nor cultural capital. With only agricultural backgrounds, involuntary minority groups lacked the skills required to take part in an industrial economy. As a result, they were locked into “quasi serfdom” (Sowell, 1981:183). Because of the large size of many involuntary minority populations and historical group conflicts with the dominant white class, market niches and “middleman minority” positions were unavailable to involuntary groups. Only with World War I, with immigration halted, were involuntary groups able to gain some industrial jobs mostly in menial, unskilled positions; and only after World War 11, through riots and the Civil Rights Movement did major gains allow for the true growth of the middle class in involuntary minority communities. Because the historical legacy of slavery, conquest, and colonization has left an enduring imprint not only on the dominant group’s view of involuntary minorities but on the psyche of the involuntary group as well, “chains and images of psychological oppression” discourage many from acting in their own best interests despite recent increases in economic and educational opportunity (Akbar, 1984; Fanon, 1967; Woodson, 1992). 34 Lacking a foundation of capital on which to build upon, the only choice left for involuntary groups has been to conform, which is inhibited by structural as well as racial barriers, or to form an “oppositional identity.” Education’s role in only offering assimilation to involuntary minorities in exchange for conformity often forces them to give up their identities and disassociate themselves from their communities (Fanon, 1967; Fordham, 1990 H and 1996; Fordharn and Ogbu, 1986; Frazier, 1962; Woodson, 1992). This is an unfair price that many are apparently unwilling to pay. In order to insure that the involuntary minority community, previously excluded from education, is made more relevant to education, while education is at the same time made more relevant to the involuntary minority community, a transformation needs to take place so that what is learned gets transmitted back to the community (Hare and Hare, 1991). Education must be given back to the people, and the increase in community control brought about through the establishment of independent, charter schools represents one possible solution. APPENDICES APPENDIX A f2pared f2ses l q f259b f2$9d APPENDIX A QUESTIONS FROM THE NATIONAL EDUCATIONAL LONGITUDINAL STUDY Parent’s highest level of education didn’t finish high school high school graduate or GED high school, some college college graduate MA. or equal Ph.D., M.D., other @Mutxwwv— Socioeconomic quartile 1 quartile 1 low 2 quartile 2 3 quartile 3 4 quartile 4 high How many times did R cut or skipped classes never 1-2 times 3-6 times 7-9 times 10- 15 times over 15 times UlrhUONr-‘O How many times got into trouble never 1-2 times 3-6 times 7-9 times 10-15 times over 15 times MuhUONi-‘O 35 36 f2325f2 Total time spent on homework out of school [per week] 0 none 1 less than 1 hour 2 1-3 hours 3 4-6 hours 4 7-9 hours 5 10- 12 hours 6 1 3-15 hours 7 16-20 hours 8 over 20 hours f2$34a Hours on weekdays R plays video games none less than 1 hour 1-3 hours 4-6 hours 7-9 hours 10- l 2 hours 13- 1 5 hours 16-20 hours over 20 hours CDVOUIéWNi-‘O f2s43 How far in school R thinks he or she will get less than high school high school only less than 2 years of school more than 2 years of school trade school degree less than 2 years of college more than 2 years of college finish college M.A. or equal Ph.D., M.D., other OOQDNONUIAWNH p—n f2367a Chances that R will graduate from high school very low low fifty-fifty high very high LII-DAWN»— 37 f2567b Chances that R will go to college very low low fifty-fifty high very high UIJEUJNH hsprgm“ The high school program in which R is currently enrolled 0 no college prep 1 college prep notdrop“ R’s dropout status 0 dropped out 1 did not drop out ‘ indicates recoded variable APPENDIX B APPENDIX B Table 1 Correlation Matrix: Socioeconomic Class and Self-Efficacy iZsesiq .9449 .905 .1555 .1515 IZSDII .000 .000 .2799 .2979 128571! .000 mg 38 APPENDIX C APPENDIX C Table 2 Correlation Matrix: Socioeconomic Class and Innovative Response Type 129:qu .1500 .000 .317? .3090 hum .000 .000 -.0175 -.0050 1259“ .237 .725 .0340 -.01 as '29“ .010 .239 .0507 am 1293" .000 .000 39 APPENDIX D APPENDIX D Table 3 Correlation Matrix: Socioeconomic Class and Academic Outcome .inarcd 1251:qu .3403 .3242 99' .000 .000 .4315 .4400 'ZZXB°""' .000 .000 .2331 .2153 “0“"0P .000 .000 4o APPENDIX E APPENDIX E REGRESSION EQUATIONS Equation Number 1 Dependent Variable: GPA GRADE POINT AVERAGE R SQUARE .36245 ADJUSTED R SQUARE .35808 GPA = .918447 + (.223501)IMMIG - (.149503)1NVOL + (.036653)F2PARED + (.038468)F2$25F2 - (.047929)F2$34A + (.006840)F2S43 + (.l9506l)F2$67A + (.066473)F2S67B - (.044500)F2S9B - (.073973)F2$9D + (.052853)F2$ESIQ + (.067981)F25EX + (.327630)HSPRGM Equation Number 2 Dependent Variable: F22XCOMP STD TEST COMP R SQUARE .42101 ADJUSTED R SQUARE .41796 F22XCOMP = 29.067628 + (2.359649)1MMIG - (1.933035)INVOL + (.882716)F2PARED + (.658061)F2$25F2 - (.617664)F2S34A + (1.979942)F2567A + (.847757)F2$67B + (l.276780)F2$ESlQ- (1.023912)F2SEX -i- (5.084597)HSPRGM Equation Number 3 Dependent Variable: NOTDROP DROP OUT STATUS R SQUARE .01091 ADIUSTED R SQUARE .01039 NOTDROP = .949854 + (.009856)F2$67A 41 BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Akbar, Na’im 1984 Chains and Images if Psychological Slavery. Jersey City, New Jersey: New Mind Productions. Alba, Richard 1995 “The Assimilation of Immigrant Groups: Concept, Theory, and Evidence.” a paper to be presented at the Conference on “Becoming American/America Becoming: International Migration to the United States,” January 18-21, 1996, sponsored by the Social Science Research Council. Benjamin, Lois 1991 The Black Elite: Facing the Color Line in the Twilight 9" the Twentieth Century. Chicago: Nelson-Hall Publishers. Blauner, Robert 1972 Racial Oppression in America. New York: Harper and Row Publishers. Bossy, E. Manchester 1970 Japanese in America. San Francisco: R. and E. Research Associates, Inc. Carrier, James G. 1986 Learning Disabiligr: Social Class and the Construction of Inequaligz in American Education. New York: Greenwood Press. Castile, George Pierre 1981 “Issues in the Analysis of Enduring Cultural Systems.” Pp. xv-xxii in George Pierre Castile and Gilbert Kushner (eds) Persistent Peoples, Tucson, Arizona: University of Arizona of Arizona Press. Chan, Sucheng and Ling-chi Wang 1991 “Racism and the Model Minority: Asian-Americans in Higher Education.” Pp. 43-67 in Philip G. Altbach and Kofi Lomotey (eds.) The Racial Crisis in American Higher Education, Albany, New York: State University of New York Press. 42 43 Cloward, Richard A. 1968 “Illegitimate Means, Anomie, and Deviant Behavior.” Pp. 156-178 in James F. Short, Jr. (Ed.) Gang Delinquengr and Delinquent Subcultures. New York: Harper and Row Publishers. Conner, John W. 1977 Acculturation and the Retention of an Ethnic Identigr in Three Generations qr [apanese Americans. San Francisco: R and E. Research Associates, Inc. Deosaran, Ramesh A. 1978 “Social Class and Academic Guidance: A Social-Psychological Analysis.” Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science 10:239-247. Du Bois, W. E. B. 1982 The Souls of Black Folk. New York: Penguin Books. Epstein, Erwin 1972 “Social Class, Ethnicity, and Academic Achievement: A Cross- Cultural Approach.” [ournal of Negro Education 41 :202-2 15. Evans, Geoffrey 1990 “Causal Explanation, Social Class, and Perceived Efficacy.” Dissertation Abstracts International 50:3079-A. 1993 “Cognitive Models of Class Structure and Explanations of Social Outcomes.” European Iournal of Social Psychology 23:445-464. Fanon, Frantz 1967 Black Skin, I/Vhite Masks. New Yorle Grove Weidenfeld. Figueroa, Peter 1991 Education and the Social Construction (f ‘Race’. New York: Routledge. Fong, Timothy P. 1994 The First Suburban Chinatown: The Remaking IfMontergr Park, California. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. 44 Fordham, Signithia 1990 “Racelessness as a Factor in Black Students’ School Success: Pragmatic Strategy or Pyrrhic Victory?” Pp. 232-262 in Nitza M. Hidalgo, Ceasar L. McDowell, and Emilie V. Siddle (eds.) Facing Racism in Education, Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard Educational Review. 1996 Blacked Out: Dilemmas of Raw, Identigl, and Success at Capital High. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Fordham, Signithia and John Ogbu 1986 “Black Students’ School Success: Coping with the Burden of ‘Acting White.’” The Urban Review 18: 176-206. Frazier, E. Franklin 1962 The Black Bourgeoisie. New Yorlc Collier Books. Gecas, Viktor and Michael L. Schwalbe 1983 “Beyond the Looking-Glass Self: Social Structure and Efficacy-Based Self-Esteem.” Social Psychology Quarterly 46:77-88. Gecas, Viktor, Michael L. Schwalbe, and Clifford L. Staples 1984 “Occupational Conditions, and Efficacy-Based Self-Esteem.” Sociological Perspectives 27:85- 109. Gibson, Margaret A. 1988 Accommodation without Assimilation: Sikh Immigrants in an American High School. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press. 1995 “Additive Acculturation as a Strategy for School Improvement.” Pp. 77-105 in Ruben G. Rumbaut and Wayne A Cornelius California’s Immigrant Children, San Diego: University of California Press. Gibson, Margaret A. and John U. Ogbu 1991 Minority Status and Schooling: A Comparative Study Of Immigrant and Involuntaol Minorities. New York- Garland Publishing, Inc. 45 Hare, Nathan and Julia Hare 1991 The Hare Plan: To Overhaul the Public Schools and Educate Every Black Man, Woman and Child. San Francisco, California: The Black Think Tank Heam, James C. 1990 “Pathways to Attendance at the Elite Colleges.” Pp. 121-145 in Paul William Kingston and Lionel S. Lewis (eds.) The High-Status Track: Studies of Elite Schools and Stratification, Albany, New York: State University of New York Press. Hing, Bill Ong 1995 “A Nation of Immigrants, A History of Nativism.” an unpublished paper presented at Harvard University, Law School. Hughes, Michael and David H. Demo 1989 “Self-Perceptions of Black Americans: Self-Esteem and Personal Efficacy.” American [ournal If Sociology 95: 132-159. Kim-Goh 1995 “Serving Asian American Children in School: An Ecological Perspective.” Pp. 145-161 in Stanley William Rothstein (ed.) Class, Culture, and Race in American Schools, Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood Press. Kingston, Paul William and Lionel S. Lewis 1990 “Undergraduates at Elite Institutions: The Best, the Brightest, and the Richest.” Pp. 105-120 in Paul William Kingston and Lionel S. Lewis (eds) The High-Status Track: Studies of Elite Schools and Stratification, Albany, New Yorlc State University of New York Press. Kluckhohn, Florence Rockwood and Fred L. Strodtbeck 1961 Variations in Value Orientations. Evanston, Illinois: Row, Peterson, and Company. 46 Light, Ivan H. 1974 “Kenjin and Kinsmen.” Pp. 283-297 in Rudolph Gomez, Clement Cottingharn, Jr., Russell Endo, and Kathleen Jackson (eds.) The Social Reality of Ethnic America, Lexington, Massachusetts: D. C. Heath and Company. Merton, Robert K. 1968 Social Theory and Social Structure. New Yorle The Free Press. Milner, Murray 1972 The Illusion q‘Equaligr: The Eflbct If Education on Opportunity, Inequaligl, and Social Conflict. San Francisco, California: Jossey-Bass, Inc., Publishers. Miller, Walter 1968 “Lower Class Culture as a Generating Milieu of Gang Delinquency.” Pp. 135-155 in James F. Short, Jr. (Ed.) Gang Delinquengz and Delinquent Subcultures. New York: Harper and Row Publishers. Min, Pyong Gap 1996 “The Entrepreneurial Adaption of Korean Immigrants.” Pp. 302-314 in Silvia Pedraza and Ruben G. Rumbaut (eds.) Origins and Destinies: Immigration, Race, and Ethnicity in America, Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Company. Miyamoto, S. Frank 1984 Social Solidarigr among the [apanese in Seattle. Seattle: University of Washington, Press. Modell, John 1977 The Economics and Politics of Racial Accommodation: The [apanese of Los Angeles, 1 900-1 942. Urbana, Chicago: University of Illinois Press. Moone, Janet R 1981 “Persistence with Change: A Property of Sociocultural Dynamics.” Pp. 228-242 in George Pierre Castile and Gilbert Kushner (eds.) Persistent People, Tucson, Arizona: University of Arizona Press. 47 Moynihan, Daniel Patrick 1965 The Negro Family: The Case for National Action. Washington, DC: US. Department of Labor. Oakes, Jeannie 1985 Keeping Track: How Schools Structure Inequality. New Haven, Connecticut: Yale University Press. Ogbu, John U. 1978 Minority Education and Caste: The American System in Cross-Cultural Perspective New Yorlc Academic Press. 1983 “Minority Status and Schooling in Plural Societies.” Comparative Education Review 27:168-190. 1987 “Variability in Minority School Performance.” Anthropology and Education Quarterly 18:312-334. 1988 “Class Stratification, Racial Stratification, and Schooling.” Pp. 163- 182 in Lois Weis (ed.) Class, Raae, and Gender in American Education, Albany, New York: State University of New York Press. Okimoto, Daniel 1974 “The Intolerance of Success.” Pp. 298-306 in Rudolph Gomez, Clement Cottingharn, Jr., Russell Endo, and Kathleen Jackson (eds.)The Social Reality of EthnicAmerica, Lexington, Massachusetts: D. C. Heath and Company. Omi, Michael and Howard Winant 1991 Racial Formation in the United States from the 1960s to the I 980s. New York: Routledge. O’Reilly, Robert P. 1970 “Racial and Social Class Isolation in the Schools: Summary Report.” a report to the Board of Regents of the University of the State of New York 48 Pedraza, Silvia 1996 “Cuba’s Refugees: Manifold Migrations.” Pp. 263-279 in Silvia Pedraza and Ruben G. Rumbaut (eds.) Origins and Destinies: Immigration, Race, and Ethnicity in America. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Company. Persell, Caroline Hodges and Peter W. Cookson, Jr. 1990 “Chartering and Bartering: Elite Education and Social Reproduction.” Pp. 25-49 in Paul William Kingston and Lionel S. Lewis (eds.) The High-Status Track: Studies of Elite Schools and Stratification, Albany, New Yorlc State University of New York Press. Portes, Alejandro 1995 “Segmented Assimilation among New Immigrant Youth: A Conceptual Framework” Pp. 71-76 in Ruben G. Rumaut and Wayne A. Cornelius California’s Immigrant Children, San Diego: University of California Press. Portes, Alejandro and Ruben G. Rumbaut 1990 Immigrant America: A Portrait. Berkeley, California: University of California Press. Rist, Ray C. 1970 “Student Social Class and Teacher Expectations: The Self-Fulfilling Prophecy in Ghetto Education.” Harvard Educational Review 40:41 1-450. Rumbaut, Ruben G. 1996 “The Crucible within: Ethnic Identity, Self-Esteem, and Segmented Assimilation among Children of Immigrants.” Pp. 1 19-170 in Alejandro Portes (ed.) The New Second Generation, New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Solomon, R. Patrick 1988 “Black Cultural Forms in Schools: A Gross National Comparison.” Pp. 249-265 in Lois Weis (ed.) Class, Race, and Gender in American Education, Albany, New York: State University of New York Press. Sowell, Thomas 1981 Ethnic America: A History. New York: Basic Books, Inc., Publishers. 49 Spener, David 1990 “Transitional Bilingual Education and the Socialization of Immigrants.” Pp. 189-209 in Nitza M. Hidalgo, Ceasar L. McDowell, and Emilie V. Siddle (eds.) Facing Racism in Education, Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard Educational Review. Suarez-Orozco, Marcelo M. 1989 Central American Refugees and U. S. High Schools: A Psychological Study of Motivation and Achievement. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Suarez-Orozco, Marcelo M. and Carola E. Suarez-Orozco 1995 “The Cultural Patterning of Achievement Motivation: A Comparison of Mexican, Mexican Immigrant, Mexican American, and Non-Latino White American Students.” Pp. 161-190 in Ruben G. Rumaut and Wayne A. Cornelius California’s Immigrant Children, San Diego: University of California Press. Takagi, Dana Y. 1992 The Retreat From Race: Asian-American Admissions and Racial Politics. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. Thomas, R Murray 1965 Social Difi'erences in the Classroom: Social-Class, Ethnic, and Religious Problems. New York- David McKay Company, Inc. Tuan, Mia 1995 “Korean and Russian Students in a Los Angeles High School: Exploring the Alternative Strategies of Two High-Achieving Groups.” Pp. 107-130 in Ruben G. Rumaut and Wayne A. Cornelius Califomia’s Immigrant Children, San Diego: University of California Press. Woodson, Carter G. 1992 The Mis-Education If the Negro. Hampton, Virginia: United Brothers and Sisters Communications Systems.