‘ a 52.35.11. . ahuufirwax .,.. , . gnu; , , . ma rsmwwfigé...” 2*! , zotw I bill. . . 5... 7...: . . Y . cvhuiL. , . u! , . . “ .VU. ’0‘ I :f. Y .i O.“ 32.3? , i. . . , , , 1 . . . . if 3 3. . . é , . .1; 3 . b...» 9,312 52.5.1»: § Hal-Hist .c f , :13} .2. A a. ,:.,.:a.l.; fifiuufiir . .1 , 45‘ s: x z .2. . . . 2.3 , , , . . . . , V 1...: A . 1.3%.: V (.393... x‘ ‘ :2 3i). 3 1 5.; n u .2. 1... r 1!, A .1. an. 1..) .11.. 3.3.1.. aura}! A?:F..J .1 .35.? 1, 4.12:. y A \x .7. J :? 7?! 3.-.! xii-v0.52... 73.. 5:11.! . {(1')}: ‘ .I.f.x).!.(\‘7 1.... Vititv‘iz; vr . ‘ . 1.: ... or . . a... Jihaln ».< :3... 24...}... r 3 1293 01051 53 89 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled IMPACT OF A DECISION SUPPORT INTERVENTION FOR MIDLIFE WOMEN ON HEALTH CARE SELF-EFFICACY presented by JILL CORRINE KROLL has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhD Psychology degree in 'Major professor MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE DI RETURN BOX to remove thi- checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE SC!" 2 5 Wj 1 i ‘3 1 s ‘ .2 .‘ fl "\ 1 MSU le An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity lnetltuion Wanna-9.1 IMPACT OF A DECISION SUPPORT INTERVENTION FOR MIDLIFE WOMEN ON HEALTH CARE SELF-EFFICACY By Jill Corrine Kroll A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1993 ABSTRACT IMPACT OF A DECISION SUPPORT INTERVENTION FOR MIDLIFE WOMEN ON HEALTH CARE SELF-EFFICACY By Jill Corrine Kroll This research examined the impact of a decision support intervention for midlife women on self-efficacy and behavior related to active participation in health care. A model of the relationship between self-efficacy, perceived barriers, outcome expectations, subjective norms, behavioral intention and participation in health care decisions was developed and tested. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three educational interventions addressing menopause and hormone replacement therapy: a written brochure, a lecture/ discussion-format intervention or a decision support intervention. Participants completed self- administered questionnaires prior to the intervention, immediately following the intervention and two months following the intervention. Self-efficacy increased significantly among participants in all three conditions following intervention. There were no significant differences in self-efficacy or self-reported active patient participation behavior between participants in the decision support intervention compared to the written brochure. Self-efficacy increased significantly more among participants in the lecture/ discussion-format intervention than among those in the written brochure condition. Significant predictors of self-reported participation in health care were intention to participate, perceived barriers to participation and outcome expectations related to active participation behavior. It was concluded that external factors such as barriers were more important to active participation behavior than were internal, individual factors such as perceived skills related to participation. Copyright by JILL CORRINE KROLL 1993 Dedicated to my patient family; John, David and Thomas who gave up so much vi ACKNOWLEDGMENTS A special thank you goes to the women who participated in the Decision Making in Menopause Study. They willingly answered my questions and provided helpful comments and insights. It is my hope that dissemination of these findings will help their concerns be heard. I want to thank my advisor, Dr. William Davidson who provided me with support, advice and helpful feedback during this long period of dissertation writing and during my years of graduate study. ' I reserve special gratitude for Dr. Marilyn Rothert, principal investigator for the Decision Making in Menopause Study. I want to thank her for serving on my dissertation committee, for letting me learn to do research under her guidance and direction and for providing me with an outstanding role model. Thank you to Dr. Neal Schmitt and Dr. Thomas Reischl for serving on my committee, encouraging me in my work, and for guiding and supporting my learning process. Thank you to the members of the Decision Making Study research team who taught me the challenge and fun of good research and collaborative effort. Thanks to Ellie, who helped me when I was down. Thanks to neighbors, friends, faculty and fellow students who provided me with help, support and diversion. Thank you to my parents, Kurt and Laurie, who believed in me even when I didn’t believe in myself and to my family, John, David and Thomas. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES .............................................................................. xiii LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................... xv CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ................................................................... 1 Statement of the Problem ....................................................... 1 Patient Participation in Decision Making ............................ 2 Psychological Morbidity .................................................. 3 Patient Preference to Participate ..................................... 5 Theoretical Models .................................................................. 6 Theory of Flamed Behavior .................................................. 6 Perceived Behavioral Control .......................................... 8 Similarity Between Social Cognitive Theory and Theory of Flamed Behavior .................................... 12 Self-Efficacy as a Mediator of Low Desire for Control of Health Care ............................................................ 15 Can Self-Efficacy be Changed? ..................................... 15 What Factors Impact Self-Efficacy? .................................... 16 Performance accomplishments...........l ................... 16 Vicarious Experience ................................................ 16 Verbal Persuasion ..................................................... 17 Physiological States ................................................... 17 Patient/ Professional Interaction ......................................... 18 Professional as Decision Maker—Advocacy Models ...................... 18 Joint Decision Making Models ...................................... 19 viii CHAPTER Page 1 Consumer as Decision Maker—Decision Support Models ................................................... 20 Critique of Decision Support Models .................... 20 Conclusion ....................................................................... 23 Community Psychology Concepts ..................................... 23 Empowerment ................................................................. 23 Individual Versus System-Level Intervention ............ 24 Individual-Level Intervention for System-Level Change ......................................... 25 Current Programs to Empower Health Care Consumers ................................................. 28 The Need for a Program Addressing Menopause ........... 30 Conclusion ............................................................................. 30 The Current Research ........................................................... 32 Hypotheses ............................................................................. 40 Hypothesis 1 .................................................................... 40 Statement of Hypothesis .......................................... 40 Summary of Related Literature ............................... 40 Link Between Intervention and Self-efficacy. 40 Link Between Self-efficacy and Intention ........ 40 Link Between Intention and Behavior ............. 41 Link Between Active Participation Behavior and Satisfaction ................................... 41 Relation to Model ...................................................... 41 Intervention and Self-efficacy ........................... 41 Self-efficacy and Behavioral Intention ............. 42 ix CHAPTER Page 1 Behavioral Intention and Behavior ................... 42 Participation and Satisfaction with Decision. 42 Hypothesis 2 .................................................................... 42 Statement of Hypothesis .......................................... 42 Summary of Related Literature ............................... 42 Outcome Expectations ........................................ 43 Subjective Norm .................................................. 44 Relationship Between 3 Variables and Intention ........................................................ 44 Relation to Model ...................................................... 44 Hypothesis 3 .................................................................... 45 Statement of Hypothesis .......................................... 45 Summary of Related Literature ............................... 45 Relation to Model ...................................................... 46 Hypothesis 4 .......................................................................... 46 Statement of Hypothesis .......................................... 46 Summary of Related Literature ............................... 46 Relation to Model ...................................................... 47 Hypothesis 5 .......................................................................... 47 Statement of Hypothesis .......................................... 47 Summary of Related Literature ............................... 47 Relation to Model ...................................................... 49 2 Method ............................................................................... 52 Setting ......................................................................... 52 Research Participants ............................................... 52 CHAPTER Page 2 Recruitment .......................................................... 52 Random Assignment .......................................... 53 Attrition ................................................................ 53 Characteristics of the Sample ............................ 56 Research Design ........................................................ 59 Power Analysis .......................................................... 59 Procedure ............................................................................... 61 Instructors ....................................................................... 61 Intervention Pilot ............................................................ 62 The Intervention .............................................................. 63 Written Brochure Intervention ................................ 63 Lecture/ Discussion Intervention ............................ 63 Decision Support Intervention ................................ 64 Attrition Prevention ........................................................ 66 Data Collection ................................................................ 67 Measures ............................................................................... 68 Instrument Pilot ............................................................... 68 Outcome Measurement ............ . ...................................... 69 Health Care Self—Efficacy ......................................... 69 Barriers to Participation Measure ........................... 73 Outcome Expectations Scale .................................... 77 Subjective Norms Measure ...................................... 79 Behavioral Intentions Measure ............................... 83 Behavior Self-Report Measure ................................ 86 Satisfaction with Decision ........................................ 89 xi CHAPTER Page 2 Scale Intercorrelations .............................................. 89 3 Results ............................................................................... 92 Hypotheses ............................................................................. 92 Hypothesis 1 .................................................................... 92 Hypotheses 2, 3 & 5 ........................................................ 98 Hypothesis 4 .................................................................. 104 Additional Analyses ............................................... 104 Summary ........................................................................ 108 4 Discussion ............................................................................ 109 Research Questions and Hypotheses ......................... 109 Summary .................................................................. 121 Limitations ..................................................................... 123 Issues for Future Research ........................................... 124 Conclusions and Recommendations .......................... 125 LIST OF REFERENCES ........................................................................ 133 APPENDICES APPENDD( A Recruitment Advertisements ...................................... 145 APPENDD( B Power Analysis Calculations ....................................... 146 APPENDD( C Intervention Outline ..................................................... 148 APPENDD( D Measures ................. ' ........................................................ 168 APPENDD( E Scale Means and Standard Deviations ....................... 183 xii Page APPENDIX F Calculation of Correction for Unreliability ............... 184 APPENDD( G Computation for Regression ....................................... 185 APPENDIX H Significance Test for Correlated r’s ............................ 187 LIST OF TABLES Table Page(s) 1 Common Constructs in Self-Efficacy Theory and Theory of Plamed Behavior .......................................... 14 2 Hypotheses ....................................................................... 50—51 3 Attrition by Experimental Group ....................................... 55 4 Demographic Characteristics of Sample ............................ 57 5 Health History and Access to Medical Care ..................... 58 6 Measures Administration Schedule ................................... 60 7 Instructor Schedule ............................................................... 62 8 Barriers to Participation Scale Corrected Item-Total Correlation ....................................................................... 74 9 Number of barriers Mentioned on Open-Ended Questions ............................................ 76 10 Outcome Expectations Scale Corrected Item—Total Correlation ....................................................................... 79 11 Subjective Norms Scale Corrected Item-Total Correlation ....................................................................... 81 12 Subjective Norms Sources of Influence on Decision Open-Ended Questions .................................................. 82 13 Behavioral Intention Scale Corrected Item-Total Correlation ....................................................................... 85 14 Behavior Self-Report Scale Corrected Item-Total Correlation ....................................................................... 88 15 Correlation Matrix ................................................................ 91 16 Contrast Coding of Nominal Experimental Group Variable ................................................................ 94 17 Regression Coefficients and Significance for Regression of Experimental Group on Self-Efficacy at Time 2 (controlling for Self-Efficacy at Time 1) ................. 18 Regression Equations for Self-efficacy Post-Intervention (Time 2) ....................................... ...... 95 ...... 97 xiv Table Page 19 Group Means for Intention, Behavior and Satisfaction ....................................................................... 97 20 Model as Proposed: t-values ............................................. 100 21 Standardized Residuals for Revised Model .................... 103 22 Revised Model: t-values ..................................................... 103 23 Regression Coefficients and Significance for Regression of Self-efficacy Time 3 on Self-efficacy Time 2 and Behavior .............................. 106 XV LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Theory of Reasoned Action (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986) ....... 7 2 Theory of Plamed Behavior (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986) ...... 9 3 Self-Efficacy Theory Model (Bandura, 1977) ..................... 11 4 Health Care Self—Efficacy Model ......................................... 39 5 Frequency Distribution Self-Efficacy Time 1 (Pm-Intervention) ........................ 70 6 Frequency Distribution Self-Efficacy Time 2 (Post-Intervention) ...................... 71 7 Frequency Distribution Self-Efficacy Time 3 (2 Months Post-Intervention)... 72 8 Frequency Distribution Barriers to Participation ................................................. 75 9 Frequency Distribution Outcome Expectations Scale .......................................... 78 10 Frequency Distribution Subjective Norm Scale .................................................... 81 11 Frequency Distribution Behavioral Intention Scale ............................................. 84 12 Frequency Distribution Behavior Self-Report Scale ............................................. 87 13 Frequency Distribution Satisfaction With Decision Time 3 ................................ 9O 14 Regression of T1 Self-Efficacy on T2 Self-Efficacy by Experimental Group ....................................................... 96 15 Path Coefficients for Model as Proposed .......................... 99 16 Path Coefficients for Revised Model ................................ 101 17 Behavior Plotted Against Self-Efficacy at Time 2 and Time 3 ......................................................... 107 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Statement of the Problem Women face a dilemma as they reach midlife. Hormone Replacement Therapy [HRT] has been found to significantly reduce white women’s risk of heart disease and osteoporosis and to decrease deaths from all causes (Cummings, Black 8: Rubin, 1989). It significantly reduces hot flashes and vaginal dryness among most women (Judd, 1987). At the same time it has been found to significantly increase risk of endometrial cancer (Persson, Adami, Lindberg, Johansson, & Manell, 1989) and has an unknown affect on risk of breast cancer (Dupont, Page, Rogers 8: Parl, 1989; Bergkvist, Adami, Persson, Hoover 8: Schairer, 1989). It may also be accompanied by side effects such as vaginal bleeding, swelling and breast tenderness (Luciano, Turksoy, Carleo 8: Hendrix, 1988). Because the decision whether or not to take hormone replacement therapy depends on individual values as well as personal risk factors, it has been argued that the patient should be included in the decision process. Although involvement of patients in the decision process has been advocated throughout public health and patient education literature for more than two decades (Steele, Blackwell, Gutmann 8: Jackson, 1987); studies have found that many patients prefer to leave medical decisions to professionals (Ende, 1989; Beisecker, 1988; Strull, Lo 8: Charles, 1984) though few studies have examined the reasons for this patient preference. The only consistent predictors of patient desire to participate in medical decisions have been age and education (Cassileth, Zupkis, Sutton-Smith 8: March, 1980; Blanchard, Labrecque, Ruckdeschel 8: Blanchard, 1988; Beisecker, 1988). These demographic factors offer little insight into the reasons behind patient preference. Preference to leave medical decisions 1 2 exclusively to professionals is puzzling when one examines the benefits associated with active participation and the hazards of lack of involvement. 'nP 'iatin' D ii Makin There are significant advantages to active patient participation in decisions. Active patients may be better able to participate in prevention, management and cure of disease resulting in reduced health care costs and decreased physical and psychological morbidity. Many illnesses today are directly related to behavior (Glanz, Lewis, 8: Rimer, 1990) and others require active patient involvement and cooperation for effective management (Giloth, 1990; McKinlay, McKinlay 8: Beaglehole, 1989). Informed health care consumers may be able to avoid or reduce illness through modifying health related behavior. A second advantage is that informed consumers may reduce the costs associated with health care. Consumers can benefit by practicing informed use of limited medical resources (Figge, 1990). Knowing effective self-treatment methods, how to apply them and when to seek expert advice can. help to decrease costs. A third benefit of patient participation in decisions is incorporation of the patient’s individual needs and values in the treatment choice. Many medical conditions have several treatment options which are equally effective (Wilson, Hart 8: Dawes, 1988). In these cases, patient participation in decision making can ensure that the treatment chosen best meets the patient’s personal needs. Furthermore, medical technology has provided new choices which demand decisions based on ethics and values rather than simply medical judgment (Saxton, 1987). For example, prenatal diagnosis makes it possible to diagnose and abort defective fetuses (Saxton, 1987). Such decision choices reflect ethical rather than clinical decisions. Society will suffer if decisions resulting from this technology do not reflect the values of health care consumers. 3 . Further justification for involving patients in decision making derives from research on self-efficacy. Studies of clinical outcome and self-efficacy have shown that self-efficacy is associated with improved functional status. Allen, Becker and Swank (1990) found that greater predischarge self-efficacy was the strongest predictor of functional status at 6 months post-surgery among coronary bypass patients. ”These findings support the belief that restoring self-confidence to perform activities is essential for recovery of the patient with heart disease,” (Allen, et al., 1990, p. 342). Davis-Berman (1989) found that self-efficacy was a better predictor of depressive symptoms among a sample of older adults than was physical illness. ”Results of this study suggest that illness is not the most salient factor, but rather, that the cognitive interpretation of physical health and functioning is the more pivotal variable in the prediction of depressive symptoms,” (Davis-Berman, 1989, p. 213). A study of an intervention to increase Active Patient Orientation among hypertension patients found that patients who were afforded a high degree of Active Patient Orientation were more likely to have their blood pressures under control and exhibited more positive cognitive and behavioral responses to illness-management (Schuhnan, 1979). Improved health outcomes related to self- efficacy suggest that the traditional patient role (clients excluded from participation in decision making) may adversely affect health outcomes. E l] .1111.“ Patients who have not had an opportunity to participate in decision making regarding their treatment may suffer increased depression and anxiety. Among 30 patients with early breast cancer, Morris 8: Royle (1988) found that a significantly higher percentage of the patients who were not offered a choice of surgery versus excision plus radiotherapy experienced clinical levels of anxiety and depression pre-operatively and up to 2 months post-operatively compared 4 to patients allowed to choose among the two treatments. Morris and Royle (1988) concluded that offering a choice of operation may reduce distress. One explanation for the observed reduction in distress may be that offering a choice in treatments mitigates the helplessness experienced by individuals who find themselves in the role of patient. The patient role encompasses a loss of control inherent to illness which is amplified by denial of the opportunity to participate in treatment choices and decisions. Repeated exposure to uncontrollable events has been found to be associated with an increase in depression and an inability to take corrective actions when events are once again within the individual’s control (learned helplessness) (Abramson, Seligman 8: Teasdale, 1978). Individuals who learn that nothing they do in the role of patient will influence which treatment they undergo, may generalize this helplessness to other aspects of the patient role such as adherence to the prescribed treatment or self-care. Increasing the patient’s sense of helplessness by denying participation in decision making may result in decreased self-efficacy for carrying out health- related behaviors and reinforce an external locus of control as it relates to health. In a review of the literature related to locus of control and health, Strickland (1978) concluded that patients who perceived that they had little control over their own health were significantly less likely to change their behavior to prevent future illness or to improve present health than were patients who perceived that they had control. Denying patients participation in health care choices may promote generalized helplessness and depression by affecting locus of control and self-efficacy. Further evidence that the loss of control experienced when an individual assumes the patient role may negatively affect health outcomes can be found in studies of control. Langer (1983) found increased mortality among nursing home patients who did not receive an intervention to induce a sense of responsibility. 5 Patients who received a talk emphasizing their responsibility for themselves and who were given plants to care for themselves had a lower mortality rate than did patients who received a talk emphasizing the staff’s responsibility for them as patients and who were given plants that were watered by the staff (Langer, 1983) Although some studies have concluded that patients do not wish to participate actively in health care decisions, midlife women may be an exception. In a study of 262 college-educated midlife women, Duffy (1988) concluded that highly educated midlife women would not want to leave their health to chance. Furthermore, although studies have found that many patients prefer to leave decisions to professionals, a significant minority prefer to make decisions themselves or together with the practitioner (Degner 8: Sloan, 1992; Strull, et al., 1984). Patient participation in health care decisions has many advantages for patients. Patients who are informed and actively involved in decision making are more likely to have the information and skills needed to maintain their health and prevent disease. Informed use of limited medical resources can decrease health care costs. Active patient involvement can ensure incorporation of patient values in medical decisions and make health care more reflective of individual patient needs and values. Patients with higher self-efficacy and active patient orientation demonstrate improved clinical outcomes and decreased I psychological morbidity. Research on learned helplessness and lack of control suggest that the passive patient role may negatively affect patient health and well-being. Finally, some patients do wish to participate actively in their health care. Given the compelling reasons for active patient involvement in health care, 6 and evidence that some patients do wish to actively participate, why do other patients prefer to leave decisions to professionals? One explanation is that individuals believe that they lack the skills necessary for effective participation. Trinkaus (1991) asked a convenience sample of 779 first-year business students (91% of whom were age 23 or below) whether they would like their physicians to provide them with more information about medications prescribed for them and whether they normally asked for this information. Trinkaus found that although 95% of American respondents (n=431) indicated that they desired more information about medications only 52% indicated that they normally asked for this information. Trinkaus speculated that patients might be hesitant to ask for desired information out of fear of offending, not being given the opportunity, not knowing how to ask, a feeling of inferiority, a desire to avoid embarrassment or because the physician behaved in an intimidating, authoritarian manner. Factors which may influence patient participation behavior then, include internal control factors such as confidence or self-efficacy and external control factors such as system-level barriers to participation (Steele, et al., 1987). Several models of behavior have been developed which use factors such as control to predict a variety of behaviors. To better understand the factors important in determining patient participation behavior an examination of these models is needed. The next section will describe two theoretical models of behavior which include the factors Trinkaus and others have suggested may be important to participation behavior. IheoneticaLMQdels. Iheomofflannedkhamz One theoretical model which may help explain patient preferences and behavior is the ”theory of planned behavior” (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). The theory of planned behavior is an extension of the (Fishbein 8: Ajzen, 1975) theory 7 of reasoned action (Figure 1). According to the theory of reasoned action, any behavior is preceded by the intention to perform that behavior. The determinants of intention are 1) attitude toward the behavior and 2) subjective norm. Attitude toward the behavior is the product of the belief that a given behavior will produce a specific outcome (behavioral belief) and the subjective value of that outcome. The subjective norm is the sum of normative beliefs. Normative beliefs are the product of a) the likelihood that another person or group would approve or disapprove of the behavior and b) motivation to comply with the other’s opinion. w BEHAVIOR Consistent with the theory of reasoned action, behavioral intentions have been found to be highly correlated with volitional behavior, and behavioral attitudes and subjective norms have been found to predict behavioral intentions (Ajzen,Timko, 8: White, 1982; Manstead,Proffitt, 8: Smart, 1983; Hinsz 8: Nelson, 8 1990). In a study of the relationship between attitudes, intentions and voting behavior, for example, Ajzen et al., (1982) found that attitude toward voting was correlated .51 (p <. 01) with intention to vote; subjective norm was correlated .35 (p < .01) with intention to vote, and intention to vote was correlated .70 (p < .01) with actual voting behavior. One limitation of the theory of reasoned action however, was that it applied only to volitional behavior (behavior which is completely under the individual’s control) (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). In order to expand the theory of reasoned action to include behavior which depends on factors other than simple will (such as time, money, skills or the cooperation of other people), the theory of planned behavior added the factor, ”perceived behavioral control” as an additional predictor of behavioral intention (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). E . 1 B l . l C l Perceived behavioral control was defined as ”the person’s belief as to how easy or difficult performance of the behavior is likely to be” (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986, p. 457). Perceived behavioral control consists of beliefs about two sources of control; internal factors and external factors. Internal factors include skills, abilities, knowledge and planning. External factors include time, opportunity, and dependence on the cooperation of other people (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). According to the theory of planned behavior, perceived behavioral control will be a significant predictor of behavioral intention beyond attitude toward the behavior and subjective norms when the behavior in question is not entirely under volitional control (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Furthermore, perceived behavioral control will contribute significantly to prediction of the behavior itself (beyond the variance explained by behavioral intention) when perceived behavioral control is an accurate reflection of actual behavioral control. Perceived 9 behavioral control is considered a partial substitute for a measure of actual control (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Behavior The theory of planned behavior is shown in Figure 2. According to the model, perceived behavior is correlated with attitude toward the behavior and subjective norm as well as with intention. The theory of planned behavior has been found to predict behavioral intention better than the theory of reasoned action when the behavior is not completely volitional (Catch 8: Kendzierski, 1990; Netemeyer 8: Burton, 1990; DeVellis,Blalock, 8: Sandler, 1990; Schifter 8: Ajzen, 1985; Beale 8: Manstead, 1991). Although the planned behavior model has been found to explain a significant degree of the variance in non-volitional behavior, some studies have found that the model fails to explain behavior as well as alternative models. Dzewaltowski, Noble and Shaw (1990) found that perceived behavioral control, subjective norm 10 and intentions failed to account for any unique variance in physical activity participation over self-efficacy. Multiple R for the theory of planned behavior model was .52 for behavioral intentions and .32 (p < .05) for prediction of physical activity behavior. Self-efficacy combined with self-evaluation of behavior contributed to significant prediction of physical activity behavior (R = .46; p > .05). Other studies have found that self-efficacy contributed significantly to predicting behavior and behavioral intentions when added to the theory of reasoned action (McCaul,O'Neill, 8: Glasgow, 1988; Tedesco,Keffer, 8: Fleck- Kandath, 1991; Dzewaltowski, 1989; Brubaker 8: Wickersham, 1990). It has been noted that the construct of perceived behavioral control (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986) is highly related to Bandura’s (1977) construct of self-efficacy expectations (Ajzen, 1988). Self-efficacy has been defined as ”the conviction that one can successfully execute the behavior required to produce the outcomes” (Bandura, 1977). Self-efficacy theory differentiates between efficacy expectations and outcome expectations. Outcome expectations are a person’s estimate that a given behavior will lead to certain outcomes (Bandura, 1977). Davis and Yates ( 1982) manipulated self-efficacy and outcome expectations among college undergraduates using an anagram task and found that performance deficits and depressive affect occurred only when self-efficacy was low and outcome expectancy was high, supporting self-efficacy theory. Figure 3 shows a diagrammatic representation of the difference between efficacy expectations and outcome expectations. According to self-efficacy theory, efficacy expectations affect people’s choice of behavioral settings, how much effort they will expend and how long they will persist in the face of obstacles. 11 -' 1B r1 PERSON —|--> BEHAVIOR —|—> OUTCOME _ __ _J _ _ _ _ .J _ _ I EFFICACY -| l OUTCOME —| IE’SEEETE'Q‘E IE’SEEETM'QNS Many studies have validated the important role of self-efficacy in behavior. Self—efficacy was found to predict persistence in pain control without medication (Manning 8: Wright, 1983); post-treatment drinking behavior (Solomon 8: Annis, 1990); and efficient use of analytic strategies in a laboratory management exercise (Bandura 8: Wood, 1989). Although self—efficacy theory has been used extensively in predicting behavior, some aspects of the theory appear to be problematic (Eastman 8: Marzillier, 1984). For example, although self-efficacy theory makes a distinction between outcome expectations and efficacy expectations, Maddux,Sherer, 8: Rogers (1982) found that an experimental manipulation designed to influence outcome expectancies, influenced expectations of self-efficacy as well. Describing an interpersonal communication technique as effective (manipulation of outcome expectations) produced an increase in self-efficacy expectations as well as outcome expectations (Maddux et al., 1982). This raised questions about the theoretical and practical distinction between the two constructs. According to self-efficacy theory, when outcomes (defined as the consequence of behavior) are inherent to a given behavior, or when people anticipate outcomes based on how well they expect to perform, outcome expectations can not be differentiated from self-efficacy expectations. In this case, 12 outcome expectations may not add to the prediction of behavior (Bandura, 1986). Williams and Kinney (1991) found that outcome expectations (anticipated pain) did not significantly add to prediction of pain tolerance behavior when self— efficacy was held constant. Although some studies have found that outcome expectations do not add to the predictability of behavior (Manning 8: Wright, 1983; Solomon 8: Annis, 1990), theorists have emphasized the value Of the distinction between the two constructs. ”Both self-efficacy and outcome expectations are likely to be important and perhaps differentially important as a function of the specific client problem,” (Kazdin, 1978, p. 180). In the theory of reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior, ”belief that a given behavior will produce a specific outcome” is one of the factors which contributes to attitude toward the behavior (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). In summary, although there are some questions about the distinction between outcome expectations and self-efficacy expectations, there is experimental and theoretical support for maintaining that the two constructs are distinct and that, at least in some situations, they contribute separately to prediction of behavior. D . o. ' 1..'l....". . ",..‘ .u :.- l. an M. 94.”. - um . «.10. Im . ., Several of the constructs which compose the theory of planned behavior are also part of self-efficacy theory. Table 1 shows the constructs in common and their definitions. The first similarity between the two theories is that they both involve an individual’s assessment of personal skills. Self-efficacy involves organizing cognitive, social and behavioral subskills into action (Bandura, 1986). In the theory of planned behavior, personal skills, abilities and knowledge are believed to contribute to perceived behavioral control (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Self-efficacy is measured by asking the individual to indicate confidence in 13 ability to perform a given behavior on a scale from 10 to 100. Perceived behavioral control has been measured by items such as, ”If I wanted to I could easily attend this class every session,” (extremely likely/ extremely unlikely) (Ajzen & Madden, 1986). ° The major difference between self-efficacy and perceived behavioral control is that in addition to measuring the individual’s assessment of personal skills and resources, perceived behavioral control measures the individual’s assessment of external factors which would facilitate or hinder the behavior. Belief about the relationship Of behavior to outcome is also an important element of both self-efficacy theory and the theory of planned behavior. Self- efficacy theory describes outcome expectation as a person’s estimate that a given behavior will lead to certain outcomes (Bandura, 1977). In the theory of planned behavior, the underlying elements of attitude toward behavior are 1) behavioral belief which links the behavior to a certain outcome, and 2) the outcome’s subjective value (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). In summary, both self-efficacy theory and the theory of planned behavior include belief about personal skills and belief about the relationship of behavior to outcome in their models of behavior. The theory of reasoned action differs from self-efficacy theory in that it posits that the immediate precursor to behavior is behavioral intention, and it includes subjective norms as a predictor of behavioral intention. Although both self-efficacy theory and the theory of planned behavior have been found to significantly predict behavior, is there any evidence that either theory may help predict patient participation in health care? Iahlil 14 0 mu 0 u- - ' zlf-Ef' -§Th=x._l010ndTh of lair- 3°h- ' Self—Efficacy Construct Planned Behavior Construct SEW i Remixed Behavior Control The conviction that one can successfully execute the behavior required to produce the outcomes (Bandura, 1977). Quicomebspectations A person’s estimate that a given behavior will lead to certain outcomes (Bandura, 1977). A person’s belief as to how easy or difficult performance of the behavior is likely to be. lnternaljagtnm contributing to perceived control mcludejkflblabflimm and planning. External factors include time, Opportunity and dependence on other people (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). l . 1 I l B l . The degree to which a person has a favorable or unfavorable evaluation of the behavior. Consists of hehaxioraLheliefs whichlimLthe WW and l , l . . l (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). 15 l- f' a: . .. u-ol. oro o _I- 'r or u o .f_I_‘-., 4 Woodward and Wallston (1987) found that among older adults desire for control in health-related situations was mediated by low health care self-efficacy. One hundred sixteen adults between 20 and 99 years old completed measures of desire for control of health care, desire for information and health care self- efficacy. Three age groups (20—39; 40—59; 60+) differed significantly on desire for control of health care and health care self—efficacy. When health care self-efficacy was used as a covariate for desire for control of health care the main effect for age was no longer significant. Desire to be actively involved in health care decisions was dependent on self-perception of competence to participate effectively. The finding that desire to participate is mediated by self-efficacy is important because it indicates an avenue through which patient involvement in decision making can be modified. WM In a critique of self-efficacy theory, Lee (1989) concluded that reliable interventions to alter efficacy expectations could not be developed until a model of the relative weighting of various types of self-efficacy information (mastery experiences, vicarious experience, persuasion and physiological information) has been developed. Previous research indicating that self-efficacy was changed through intervention shows that this conclusion was unwarranted. Vallis and Bucher (1986) increased self-efficacy for pain tolerance during a cold pressor test. Maddux, Sherer, and Rogers (1982) successfully manipulated self-efficacy for assertiveness using verbal persuasion and Davis 8: Yates ( 1982) experimentally induced high and low self-efficacy expectations among a group of 108 male and female undergraduate students. Sanna (1992) experimentally manipulated self- efficacy expectancy by providing false feedback on a vigilance task and by manipulation of task difficulty (providing mastery experiences). 16 Although interventions can not be developed based on quantitative formulas of specific amounts of persuasion, modeling and mastery, effective interventions can be developed and tested. Interventions to increase self-efficacy should provide one or more of the four types of information outlined by Bandura (1982). t r a1 -E ' ? Bandura (1977) described four principal sources of efficacy information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. W. Performance accomplishments are the most influential sources of information (Bandura, 1977, Barling 8: Snipelisky, 1983). Success experiences raise efficacy expectations and repeated successes develop strong efficacy expectations which reduce the impact of failure experiences. Performance accomplishments are promoted through skill-building activities such as participant modeling, performance exposure and self-instructed performance. Skill-building leads to increased success experiences and strengthens efficacy expectations. Davis and Yates (1982) found that providing college students with either mastery or failure experiences in solving anagrams produced changes in selfiefficacy. Students who were given difficult anagrams had lower self-efficacy than those given easy anagrams. Participant modeling of skills like question-asking and decision making may effectively increase participant health care self-efficacy. MW. Self-efficacy may also be influenced through observation of live or symbolic models (Bandura, 1977). Schunk and Hanson (1985) found that children who observed a peer model developed higher self- efficacy for learning than those who observed a teacher model or no model at all. Observation of peers modeling communicatiOn and decision making skills in a group intervention setting may strengthen gains in health care self-efficacy 17 obtained through participant modeling and provide independent efficacy information. Managing Efficacy expectations can be increased through persuasion although expectations induced this way are easily extinguished by disconfirming experiences (Bandura, 1977). Maddux, et al., (1982) manipulated self-efficacy among introductory psychology students using verbal persuasion. Students who read essays describing a technique as easy to use had higher self-efficacy for using the technique than those who read essays describing the technique as difficult to use. Verbal persuasion can be used in health care settings to introduce new information which may change expectations and encourage participant modeling experiences. W5, Emotional arousal is another source of efficacy information (Bandura, 1977). Anxiety may be interpreted as a sign of an inability to perform in a given situation and can operate to inhibit performance of efficacious behavior. These four sources of efficacy information can be utilized to modify self- efficacy. The most effective source of efficacy information has been found to be performance accomplishments (Bandura, 1982). Following this, vicarious experience and verbal persuasion have also been found to be effective in modifying efficacy expectations (Bandura, 1982). But how can self-efficacy expectations be maintained in instances where the individual must interact with others? This issue is of particular concern related to health care self-efficacy. What health care settings are likely to promote health care self-efficacy? How can patients be trained to interact with the health care system to maximize their success experiences? What is the impact of cultural role expectations and the interaction between patients and health care providers on patient participation behavior and satisfaction? 18 Pti Prf inltrain The most significant aspect of health care settings is the interaction of the health care professional with the patient. Three basic models have been used to describe the interaction between patients and medical practitioners; 1) Professional as Decision Maker, 2) Joint Decision Making and 3) Consumer as Decision Maker. All three models have potential value in health care. For example, the professional as decision maker model may be appropriate in instances in which the patient is incapacitated or otherwise unable to participate in decision making (Schain, 1980). Below each model is described and critiqued as to its potential impact on patient health care self-efficacy. EE'IDHIH—El “I! In the advocacy model of professional / client interaction, the patient is regarded as helpless and dependent (Schain, 1980). The professional maintains an authoritative role and solves problems and makes decisions on the patient’s behalf (Sharf, 1988; Schain, 1980). In this kind of an interaction it would be difficult for a patient to develop or maintain confidence in regard to health matters. The patient would have to remold the nature of the interaction in order to take an active role in decisions. In American society, physicians in particular, though to some extent other health professionals as well, control medical knowledge, medical practice and the institutions of medicine, (Navarro, 1983). As a consequence, the medical profession commands high social and economic status (Ehrenreich 8: Ehrenreich, 1978; Navarro, 1983) which makes it difficult and unlikely that a patient could effectively change the nature of the interaction between herself and the professional (Roter, 1987). Modern medical technology further complicates the patient’s efforts at self- efficacy in health care. As medical technology becomes increasingly more 19 complex and formidable it further removes the patient from an active role. Expert technicians are required to operate and interpret the information from sophisticated equipment. Professionals and patients alike may be so fascinated with medical technology (Figge, 1990) that they treat the machines as decision makers. Davis-Floyd (1987) reported that obstetricians for example, were likely to perform a caesarean if a fetal monitor indicated a potential problem although caesarean rates have not been found to be related to neonatal outcomes. Patients may find the array of machinery and technology so daunting that they surrender all hope of ever being able to understand the nature of their health problems, let alone make decisions regarding them. The professional-as- decision-maker model provides no aid to the patient in attempting to overcome this intimidation and take an active role. 1.”: .. Ill. 1111 The second model which has described patient/ professional interactions is the joint decision making model. Schain (1980) and Roter (1987) advocated models of interaction in which patient and professional shared responsibility for health care decisions equally. The patient maintained autonomy and dignity while respecting the skill, opinions and expertise of the professional. Joint decision making models were designed to mitigate the tendency of professionals to emphasize medicotechnical concerns and neglect psychosocial issues (Roter, 1987). Psychosocial concerns of the patient are incorporated into decisions through the patient’s input into the process. Many patients may be unable to participate equally in decisions with professionals however, due to the social power differences between themselves and the professional. Joint decision models lack a strategy for supporting patients in the decision making role and fail to address the issue of power inequity between clients and professionals. Because of the difference in power between patients and professionals it is 20 likely that in practice the professional would dominate a joint relationship between clients and professionals. Joint decision making models would become professional-as-decision maker models in practice. ' i — ' i M 1 In contrast to the joint decision making models, several articles have outlined models which specifically address the consumer’s role as decision maker. Corcoran (1988) delineated the role of an advocate in the decision process. Corcoran (1988) operationalized Gadow’s 5 steps in assisting a patient in decision making in the following way: (1) Assure relevant information, (2) Enable the patient to select information, (3) Encourage patients to seek information, (4) Offer to develop a decision flow diagram, (5) Disclose nurse’s view, (6) Share personal values and viewpoints (7) Help patient determine own values, (8) Discuss the meaning of the experience for the patient. O’Connor and O’Brien-Pallais (1989) also developed a model for assisting patient’s in making decisions. O’Connor 8: O’Brien-Pallais established that the goal of decision counseling should be for the client to make an ”effective” decision . An effective decision was defined as a decision which was informed, consistent with personal values and congruent with subsequent behavior (O’Connor 8: O’Brien-Pallais, 1989). Steps in decision therapy include: (1) Clarify goals, alternatives and outcomes, (2) Realign unrealistic alternatives, (3) Identify viable alternatives, (4) Clarify outcome expectations, (5) Realign unrealistic outcome expectations, (6) Clarify desirability of possible outcomes, (7) Clarify priority of outcomes, (8) Identify values tradeoffs, (9) Facilitate alternative selection, (10) Teach self-help skills required for behavioral implementation of decision. WW. Effective decision counseling has been found to be associated with decreased levels of post-decisional regret and _ 21 increased satisfaction. Cooper, Bledin, Brice, and Mackenzie (1985) found that women who indicated regret or uncertainty about their decision to be sterilized were significantly more likely to report that their preoperative counseling had not been adequate than were women who reported that they were definitely glad they had been sterilized (p < .005). Parents who recalled being involved in making choices regarding their newborns with myelomeningocele were significantly more likely to be satisfied with the tertiary-care center than were parents who did not recall involvement in making choices (p < .01) (Charney, 1990). Decision support models of client—professional interaction have several advantages over the traditional interaction model and the joint decision making model. Both the Corcoran (1988) and the O’Connor 8: O’Brien-Pallais (1989) models define specific steps for facilitating client decisions. Unlike the joint decision making models, the decision support models emphasize that the final decision should rest with the client, with the professional’s role being to serve as facilitator in the client’s decision process. Because the emphasis is on helping clients to make decisions themselves, many of the problems associated with authoritative and shared decision models—such as the power inequity between professional and client—are circumvented. Several issues remain to be addressed, however. Control of information by the health professional has not been adequately addressed in decision support models. Corcoran (1988) noted that the professional may subtly manipulate a patient’s decision by the information that is or is not provided. Corcoran’s model left this problem open because the professional holds the responsibility for ”assessing” the amount of information the patient desires and ”can cope with.” The O’ Connor 8: O’Brien-Pallais (1989) model gave no such responsibility to the professional thus making such an occurrence less likely, though still 22 possible. Corcoran (1988) indicated that it was important to assess the patient’s desire to know and provide only the desired level of information. The clinician must guesshow much information the client desired and withhold other information. Clinicians have been found to estimate patient preferences for information inaccurately. Strull, et al. (1984) found that while 55% of patients being treated for hypertension indicated that they preferred ”Quite a lot” or ”Very Extensive” discussion about therapy, their clinicians estimated that only 43% desired that extent of discussion. The professional should communicate to the client that several levels of information are available so that the client can choose what information he or she seeks. A drawback of decision support models has been a lack of operationalized evaluation criteria. Corcoran (1988) gave no criteria by which to evaluate the effectiveness of the decision advocacy guidelines but did encourage nurses to test and refine the model and to develop additional guidelines. O’Connor 8: O’Brien- Pallais (1989) listed expected outcomes to be achieved by patients through decision support therapy. Some of the expected outcomes such as ”identifies viable alternatives” can be measured using existing measures such as decision analysis. Other outcomes such as ”expresses satisfaction at having made the best decision under the circumstances” presently lack reliable measures. Because decision support models place decision making with the client, interactions based on these models are most likely to promote health care self- efficacy. Health care self-efficacy is promoted two ways in decision support; through mastery and persuasion. First, the client experiences competence in the decision making process. Second, the professional communicates confidence in the client’s ability to make a decision which is best for him or her. 23 Conclusion Of the three models of client-professional interaction commonly advocated, the decision support model is most likely to promote high health care self- efficacy among patients. Although the model has been developed there have been few programs which have applied the model in actual practice. Despite growing demand for active involvement of patients in their health care, practical guidelines for supporting the client in this role and evaluating such efforts have been lacking. Also lacking has been a theoretical basis for evaluating interventions designed to facilitate active patient participation in medical decision making (Steele, et al., 1987). MW Theoretical concepts and tools useful for evaluation of patient participation interventions may be drawn from community psychology. Community psychology is concerned with development and evaluation of social programs. It embraces an approach to these problems which is 1) democratic, 2) innovative and 3) can be disseminated (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). Inherent to the concept of democratic change is empowerment (Rappaport, 1987). Full participation in social change requires providing information and skills to those who have traditionally been excluded from decision making. Intervention for social change can occur at the individual, group, organizational or societal levels but most often requires intervention at all four levels (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). Empowerment and level of change are two principals of community psychology which apply to evaluation of a patient participation intervention. Emmment Empowerment refers to individual determination over one’s own life and democratic participation in the life of one’s community (Rappaport, 1987). Empowerment describes both a psychological sense of personal control or 24 influence and a concern with actual social influence, political power and legal rights (Rappaport, 1987). It applies to individuals as well as to organizations (Rappaport, 1987). Empowerment has been described as a multi-factor construct which includes the elements self-efficacy, political efficacy, mastery, desire for control, and locus of control as well as others (Zimmerman 8: Rappaport, 1988). 1 Health care self-efficacy is one element of psychological empowerment related to health care. Measurement of this construct can be used to evaluate the extent to which an intervention serves to empower health care consumers in their interactions with the health care system. Self-efficacy focuses on the individual aspect of empowerment while measures of barriers or external perceived control focus on the health care system. Combined, self-efficacy and perceived barriers represent the two main elements of psychological empowerment related to health care. Fairweather and Davidson, (1986) indicated that while intervention for social changecould occur at any of four levels, it was most likely to result in successful and lasting social change if it occurred at multiple levels. Although intervening at multiple levels is most likely to promote effective change, it is difficult to do. Attempts to intervene at the system level often meet with barriers. Primary among the barriers is the tendency of those invested in the status quo to perpetuate the status quo at all costs (Fairweather 8: Tomatzky, 1977). Gray, Doan and Church (1991) reported that one barrier to empowerment of persons with cancer was the behavior of health professionals. The health professionals were invested in the traditional patient / professional model. Professionals derived social and economic status from their role as decision makers (Ehrenreich 8: Ehrenreich, 1978; Navarro, 1983). Persons with cancer 25 found it difficult to obtain the needed information from these health professionals in order to participate in decisions. - n ' f t - n One solution to the barriers to intervening at the system level is intervention at the individual level which stimulates individuals to demand changes at the system level. An example of this was the alternative birth movement in the United States. The movement was a reaction by women against the medical establishment which controlled Obstetrical care (Mathews 8: Zadak, 1991). Mathews and Zadak (1991) reviewed the history of the alternative birth movement: The natural childbirth movement gained momentum in the 1960’s and 1970’s when it converged with the feminist and consumer movements of the time. The movement emphasized public education, informed choice and self- help. Educated, middle class Americans began to choose home births and alternative birthing centers over hospital births. They demanded a more personalized, family-centered, patient-controlled birthing experience. Hospitals and obstetricians experienced a loss of control and increased competition for patients. As a result health professional organizations joined together to endorse a philosophy Of family-centered birthing and hospitals began to offer in-hospital combined labor-delivery-recovery birthing rooms. Routine procedures such as shaving the perineum, giving the laboring woman enemas and confining her to bed began to fall into disfavor. Hospital rooms were redesigned to minimize the appearance of technological intervention in birthing. The alternative birth movement initiated a major transformation in the birthing process in the United States (Mathews 8: Zadak, 1991). This transformation was a result of empowering health care consumers rather than directly intervening at the system level. Despite outward appearances however, the degree of actual change in 26 Obstetric practice can be questioned. MatheWs and Zadak (1991) pointed out that alternatives to traditional medical management of childbirth are available only to highly motivated, well-prepared women who anticipate a completely normal pregnancy and delivery. The control and prerogatives of the obstetrical community continue to be maintained. In spite of continued control by the obstetrical community however, childbirth in the United States continues to change to meet consumer demands. As hospitals have changed and consumers have continued their demands for family-centered care, medical education has slowly begun to adapt. Pregnancy and childbirth is being redefined as a natural rather than a medical event. The impact of the alternative birthing movement continues to mold US obstetrical practices. Providing health care consumers with the information and skills to challenge the traditional model of care successfully promoted change within the system. Intervention at the individual level resulted in system-level change despite resistance from those invested in the current system. Another way in which system-level change may be effected is through development, evaluation and dissemination of programs which empower health care consumers to participate actively in health care decisions. Once an effective method of empowering consumers is identified it may be disseminated to the health care community as an alternative model for interacting with patients. Dissemination involves four phases; approach, persuasion, activation and diffusion (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). The approach phase involves identifying the target population (such as individual health professionals, clinics or health maintainence organizations) and implementing a plan to inform them of the innovation (such as advertisements, professional training workshops, articles in professional journals, etc.). Persuasion involves identifying and utilizing an effective method of information transmission. It is important to 27 consider the style of persuasion, content of the message and legitimacy of the advocate (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). The most effective method of persuasion can be identified through implementation of multiple methods and evaluation of their success. Use of respected fellow professionals as advocates and focusing the content Of the message on time- and cost-efficiency as well as patient well-being are possible aspects of persuasion with health professionals. The next phase of dissemination is the activation phase. Once the professional or organization decides to adopt the innovative program it is necessary to activate the model in the community setting through accurate model replication. Activating the model in the community while maintaining its integrity requires transmission of content and process, guidance during implementation and Ongoing monitoring of implementation (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). Activating a program to empower health care consumers to participate in health care decisions will require packaging the program materials in a manner appropriate to the private office or clinic setting, appropriate training of program personnel and observation and feedback regarding the program implementation. The final phase of dissemination involves spreading the innovation from the initial adopters to the rest of the target population and society as a whole (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). During diffusion it is critical to continue monitoring implementation of the program to maintain its integrity and effectiveness. With patient empowerment programs it is imperative to avoid cooptation by the status quo such as partially occurred in the instance of the natural birth movement. Integrity of the empowerment goal of the program may be maintained by tracking adoption of the program through copyright of materials and visible requests to be informed of program adoption. Tracking Of implementation will allow program developers to closely monitor implementation and provide feedback regarding congruence with original goals. 28 The specific method used for each of the four phases of dissemination should be determined through experimentation (Fairweather 8: Davidson, 1986). rr r E H a1 ar n r Two areas in which the patient empowerment approach has been used are cancer care and diabetes education. Involvement of patients in decision making is especially important in diabetes education and in cancer care because in diabetes care the patient is personally responsible for carrying out so much of the treatment program (Anderson, Funnell, Barr, Dedrick, 8: Davis, 1991) and in cancer, patients lose much control in their lives (Gray, et al., 1991). The University of Michigan Diabetes Research and Training Center developed a training program aimed at teaching diabetes educators to empower patients. The program included four steps; 1) Help patients determine what part of their diabetes care is a problem for them, 2) Help patients focus on their emotions, 3) Help patients clarify their health-related values and establish goals, and 4) Help patients develop and commit to a specific plan to achieve their goals (Anderson, et al., 1991). Participating professionals were required to follow a simulated diabetes care regimen for 3 days prior to participation in the skills- based workshop. The goal of the simulation was to introduce the professionals to the challenges of caring for diabetes on a daily basis. The program was based on Rogers’ client-centered counseling model and was developed, pilot tested and offered to diabetes educators. Evaluation of the effectiveness of the program in empowering patients was not reported. A second program focused on cancer patients. Because people diagnosed with cancer lose control in their lives due to illness, side effects and unbalanced power relations with health professionals, they can benefit from interventions to empower them in relation to the health care system (Gray, et al., 1991). Gray, et al. (1991) developed a model to empower people diagnosed with cancer. The 29 model included the following ways Of empowering people: 1) Address symptom management for persons with cancer, 2) increase funding for outpatient, community and home-based care, 3) allow patients to determine the amount of information and participation they receive 4) teach patients stress-management techniques, 5) teach assertiveness training for patients, 6) develop mutual support groups for people with cancer, 7) involve patients in setting health care policies, 8) hire patient advocates, 9) encourage political activism by persons with cancer, 10) change societal attitudes toward illness and death to decrease stigmatization of cancer survivors. Both programs addressed patient empowerment on two levels, change at the system level and change directed toward patients themselves. At the system level, the diabetes education program sensitized diabetes educators to the problems of diabetes patients by having them follow a diabetic care regimen for 3 days. It educated professionals in how to change their behavior in order to empower patients. The model for empowering persons with cancer recommended involving patients in setting health care policies and encouraging political activism. . Both programs developed to empower medical patients also included elements directed at intervening with patients themselves. The individual-level elements of the interventions were similar to those advocated in decision support models of client-professional interaction.The diabetes program trained educators to intervene with patients using the four steps described above. The model for persons with cancer emphasized informing patients and training them in skills such as stress-management and assertiveness. The four recommendations shared by the individual-level portions of the intervention programs and by the decision support models of client-professional interaction were 1) provide information about the factors important to the health- 30 related decision; 2) help the client clarify his or her values related to the important factors; 3) involve the client in health care decisions; and 4) help the client develop and commit to a specific plan to carry out health care decisions. These four elements along with system level interventions, form the basis of the patient participation philosophy. Although there have been programs aimed at empowering patients with specific medical problems, no program has combined the four basic elements of patient participation into an intervention to aid women in their decision making related to menopause which is the focus of the research reported in this paper. Th N f r r A ' M Menopause is a time of physical changes for women. Recent research demonstrating benefits and risks associated with hormone replacement therapy after menopause contribute to the decision making dilemmas women face at menopause. Because women at midlife face physical changes associated with menopause as well as new concerns about health changes associated with aging, the need to be an informed decision maker regarding health issues is especially important. Additionally, it is important to involve women in health care decisions at menopause because they are ultimately responsible for carrying out the care plan selected. The woman herself must carry out many of the activities which may benefit her health such as exercise programs, diet changes or taking medicines as directed. Because of the changes and decisions women face at menopause a program which promotes their participation in their health care is needed. mm Women at midlife face an increased risk of health problems such as osteoporosis and heart disease. They may choose to respond to these increased risks through health promotion behaviors such as regular exercise, elimination of 31 smoking and diet modifications. They may also choose whether or not to take hormone replacement therapy. Because the issues related to health maintainence and hormone replacement therapy are complicated and controversial, midlife women may benefit from informed, active involvement in decision making related to their health care. Active consumer participation in health care is important for other reasons as well. Expanded choices and increased costs of medical care provide medical practitioners with multiple treatment options. Consumers should help choose the option which best represents their own values. Furthermore, active participation and a sense of self-efficacy related to health have been found to result in improved clinical outcomes and decreased psychological morbidity. Despite potential benefits related to active participation, health care consumers frequently decline to participate in health care decisions. One explanation is they believe they lack skills necessary for effective participation. Health care consumers have low self-efficacy related to health care. One solution to this problem is to increase health care self-efficacy. It has been demonstrated that self-efficacy can be modified through intervention. Bandura (1977) described four principal sources of efficacy information through which intervention could occur; performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, persuasion and physiological states. An intervention which enhanced self-efficacy through these sources could promote participation among individuals for whom low self-efficacy was a barrier. Health care self-efficacy may also be modified by the quality of the interaction between the consumer and the health care professional. Interactions in which the professional maintains control over decision making may serve to lower consumer self-efficacy. Alternatively, interactions which promote autonomy by the consumer may serve to increase self—efficacy. The advocacy 32 and the joint decision making models of interaction may hinder development of health care self-efficacy by encouraging a passive role for the consumer. In the advocacy model the professional makes decisions for the patient. In the joint decision making model the professional may encourage input from the patient but the status contrast between patient and professional may prevent active participation by the patient. The decision support model of interaction is hypothesized to have the most positive impact on health care self-efficacy. In this model the health care consumer is assisted to make decisions which are informed, consistent with values and congruent with subsequent behavior (O’Connor 8: O’Brien-Pallais, 1989) Few programs to empower health care consumers have been developed and evaluated. Two programs which have been developed incorporated aspects of the decision support model of interaction. These programs 1) informed consumers, 2) helped the consumer clarify values, 3) promoted consumer decision making, and 4) helped the consumer develop a personal plan to carry out health care decisions. Although a few programs have been developed to empower health care consumers to participate actively in decision making, no program has been developed and evaluated which addressed the issue of menopause. Development and evaluation of such a program is needed. Concepts and methods from community psychology may be useful in evaluating such a program. The concepts of empowerment and multi-level intervention may be applied to determine the effectiveness of such a program in empowering midlife health care consumers. The Current Research The purpose of the current research was to determine the impact of three 33 menopause education programs on the health care self-efficacy of midlife women and to test a model of the impact of self-efficacy on participation in health care decisions. Because self—efficacy has been found to be related to desire for control of health care and it has been documented that some patients desired more information than they actively requested from health care providers, it was hypothesized that patient participation behavior could be influenced by increasing self-efficacy related to active participation behavior. The three intervention programs to be tested for their impact on self-efficacy were developed by Rothert (1990) to help women become better decision makers about their menopausal health and hormone replacement therapy. The first program, (A) provided information in the form of a wrifien brochure. The brochure consisted of three sections addressing 1) the physiology of menopause and self-care, 2) the pros and cons of hormone replacement therapy and 3) communication with health care professionals (Rothert, Kroll, Holmes-Rovner, Rovner, Schmitt, 8: Talarczyk, 1992). Section one of the brochure described what women could expect at menopause, including information on what menopause is, when it occurs and the physical changes women may experience at menopause. Section one discussed the symptoms and increased risks that may accompany menopause and suggested self-care strategies. Section two of the brochure described additional risk factors to consider related to use of hormone replacement therapy and described the two main types of hormone therapy; estrogen-only therapy and estrogen combined with progesterone. The benefits and risks of each type of therapy and the increase or decrease in death rate due to endometrial cancer, heart disease and osteoporosis were depicted numerically and graphically. Section three of the brochure was in workbook format with spaces to record personal and family health history, menstrual history and questions for the 34 health care professional. Examples illustrated how to provide detailed information to help health professionals care for one’s health and sample questions were offered to help the reader think of her own questions and concerns related to menopause. Recommendations for communicating effectively during the visit included the following: describe symptoms specifically, let the professional know about questions at the beginning of the visit (to allow time during the visit to have them answered), ask questions and ask for clarification when necessary, repeat information to ensure understanding and write down information that will be needed later. 1 ' The brochure was designed as a standard treatment control for the decision support intervention (described below). Although the brochure provided instruction in effective communication with health care professionals, it was not anticipated that the brochure alone would be sufficient intervention to increase self-efficacy related to the health care system. The second program (B) provided information in a W format. Program B consisted of three 1 1/ 2 hour sessions using a lecture format with overhead transparencies as visual aids. Program content was parallel to the brochure. Session I covered the physiology of menopause and self-care methods for managing symptoms and decreasing health risks. Session II discussed the pros and cons of hormone replacement therapy and Session III covered communicating with health care providers. A 10 to 20 minute question and answer period was led by a nurse or physician member of the instructor team following discussion of the pros and cons of HRT. Session III presented a lecture on communicating with health care providers which paralleled the content of Section III of the brochure but instructors included anecdotal examples to illustrate how effective communication could result in more satisfying interactions with the health care system. For example, an advisory panel of 35 health professionals indicated that it would be helpful for patients to convey that they have questions at the beginning of a visit in order to allot time to answer them. The reasoning behind this recommendation was therefore explained in the lecture, but not in the written brochure. The lecture/ discussion format program was not anticipated to significantly impact self-efficacy related to the health care system. Although information on effective communication with health care providers was presented verbally as well as in written form, instruction alone was not expected to be an effective intervention by itself. The lecture / discussion program was designed as a time and attention control group for the decision support intervention. The third program (C) was a decision support intervention which provided information and experience in an MW]; format. The intervention consisted of a series of exercises to foster active involvement in the decision process. The program, parallel to Program B in time spent, consisted of three sessions, each approximately 1 1/ 2 hours long. The first session of the program was identical in content to Programs A and B. Information on the physiology of menopause and self-care strategies for managing symptoms and minimizing health risks was presented in a lecture format. The second session of Program C presented the information contained in the brochure and in Program B on the risks and benefits of hormone replacement therapy. As in Program B, a question and answer period was included following presentation of the pros and cons of hormone replacement therapy. The women received information about a previous study by the research team which found that different women weighed the risk factors related to HRT in differing ways (Rothert, et al., 1990). This information was presented to help the women understand that women differed in how they approached the decision about 36 HRT and to introduce an exercise assessing personal risks and values related to HRT. In order to better understand their own risks and values related to HRT the women completed a Personal Risk Assessment, Problem Significance Assessment and Relevance Chart which combined base rate risk, personal risk and personal values for each of up to 7 factors important to the woman’s decision about HRT. The Personal Risk Assessment informed the women of their personal risk of heart disease, osteoporosis and endometrial cancer. The women scored their own risk assessments and marked base rate risk and their personal risk on their relevance charts. The purpose of the relevance chart was to help women visually compare their risks and values across the multiple factors in their decision. Following the Personal Risk Assessment exercise, the women completed the Problem Significance Assessment on which they rated having each of the following factors on an 11 point scale with 0=Perfect Health and 100=Death: fractures from osteoporosis, heart disease, endometrial cancer, hot flashes, cyclic bleeding, treatment side effects and other. Intermediate points on the scale were marked in 10-point increments from 10 to 100. The Problem Significance Assessment was designed to help the women clarify their values related to the tradeoffs in the HRT decision. This information was transferred in graphic form to the Relevance Chart. After completion of the Personal Risk Assessment and Problem Significance Assessment, the women were given a work sheet with a series of six scenarios describing various combinations of risks and values. One or two of the scenarios were read in class and sample relevance charts were completed on overhead transparencies to show how a woman might weigh and combine the information and come to a decision. The women were then given time to read over the other scenarios, placing themselves in each situation, and consider how they would 37 weigh the various factors and make a decision related to symptom management and risk minimization. Following the scenarios exercise, the women returned to the relevance chart and assigned a ”relevance” score to each factor based on her risk and significance for that factor (heart disease, fractures, endometrial cancer) or significance alone. The relevance chart visually depicted each woman’s unique combination of risks and values with base rate information. The women weighed the relevance of each factor to make a preliminary choice between 1) I choose not to discuss HRT with my health care provider, 2) I choose to discuss taking HRT with my health care provider or 3) I choose to continue to take HRT. During session III of the decision support intervention, participants received the same lecture on communicating with health care providers presented to participants in program B. The information was parallel to that provided in the brochure and included anecdotal examples to support the value of effective communication and participation in health care. In addition, participants took part in a role play exercise in which they took turns acting as health care providers and as active patients. Participants received the following instructions prior to the role play, ”For those of you who are playing health care providers, remember you have a schedule to keep and patients in the waiting room but you are seeing one of your regular patients. For those of you who are playing patients, you have called ahead and informed the receptionist you want to make an appointment to discuss menopause.” The purpose of this exercise was to provide a participant modeling experience in which the women could gain confidence actively participating in a simulated health care encounter. The women separated into pairs and took turns acting as the health professional and as the patient. In addition to gaining experience and confidence in active patient behavior, this allowed participants to gain understanding of how a health care 38 provider might feel when facing an active patient. Following the role play there was a discussion period in which the instructor asked the group the how the role play went and how it worked. One comment included that when playing the health care provider it felt uncomfortable not to know the answers to the I patient’s questions. This allowed a brief discussion about whether health care providers may know the answers to all questions. Sometimes discussion focused on past experiences in actual health care encounters and how the concepts learned in the class could be applied to the situation in the future. The purpose of the role play was to increase self-efficacy related to active participation in health care, which was expected to lead to increased active behavior in health care encounters. Although mastery experiences have been found to be the most effective in increasing self-efficacy, practical limitations required that the intervention use a simulated rather than actual health care encounter. The hypothesized impact of the intervention on health care self-efficacy and participation behavior is depicted in Figure 4. Because mastery experiences have been found to be the strongest sources of self-efficacy information, it was hypothesized that intervention C, which included experience in a simulated health care encounter, would significantly increase self-efficacy. The brochure alone and the lecture/ discussion format were not expected to significantly increase self-efficacy related to the health care system. The relationship between the hypotheses and the proposed model are described below. 39 EiguLeA. Attitude Toward the Behavior E: . . Intention E Subjective to a) Norms Partici ate 2 (31) 22 .5- Behavior Outcome _4 Perceived Behavioral Control (7) 40 Hypotheses Hypothesis 1 W Post-intervention, participants in Program C will have greater confidence in their ability to participate actively in health care decisions which will result in greater intention to participate, increased participation behavior and greater satisfaction with decision compared to participants in programs A and B. SummaryoLBolafedLiteratoro WM. In the Health Care Self-Efficacy model (figure 4), Intervention C (1) was hypothesized to impact the self-efficacy aspect of perceived behavioral control (2) through participant modeling and persuasion. Since participant modeling has previously been found to increase self-efficacy among snake phobics (Bandura, Adams, 8: Beyer 1977), it was hypothesized that the role play exercise would increase participants’ health care self-efficacy in a similar way. The brochure alone and the lecture/ discussion format intervention were not expected in significantly influence self-efficacy. WW. It was hypothesized that the increase in self-efficacy (2) induced by Intervention C (1) would result in increased intention to actively participate in health care decisions (3). The impact of self-efficacy on behavioral intention has been well documented. For example, self-efficacy was found to be predictive of intention to perform dental hygiene behaviors (McCaul, O’Neill 8: Glasgow, 1988; Tedesco, Keffer 8: Fleck- Kandath,1991) and intention to participate in physical activity (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990). In previous studies, the correlation between self-efficacy and behavioral intention was found to be substantial; .89 for teeth-brushing (Tedesco, Keffer 8: Fleck-Kandath,1991) and .81 for physical activity (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990). 41 Wm. Because previous studies have found that behavioral intention was a significant predictor of behavior, it was hypothesized that intention to participate actively in health care (3) would predict self-reported participation in health care (4). Intention has been found to predict class attendance among college students (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986), brushing and flossing among dental patients (Tedesco, Keffer 8: Fleck-Kandath, 1991), physical activity participation among undergraduate college students (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990), and testicular self-examination among college students (Brubaker 8: Wickersham, 1990). ii fa a o a . The outcome of active participation in health care (4) was hypothesized to be satisfaction with decision (5). Parents who recalled being involved in the decision making regarding their hospitalized children were significantly more likely to be satisfied with their care than were parents who did not recall involvement (Chamey, 1990). It was therefore anticipated that patients who actively participated in their health care would be more satisfied with the health care decision made. RolafionJoModel Hypothesis 1 regarded the relationship between intervention C (1), self- efficacy (2), intention to participate (3), participation in health care (behavior) (4) and satisfaction with health care decision (outcome) (5). W. The health care self-efficacy model shown in Figure 4 shows the hypothesized relationship between the intervention (1) and self-efficacy (2) suggested by the literature. The intervention was hypothesized to increase self-efficacy through participant modeling and persuasion. In the model, the factors self-efficacy (2) and perceived barriers (6) paralleled Shifter 8: Ajzen’s (1985) construct ”perceived behavioral control (7).” Self-efficacy expectations 42 represented the internally controlled aspects of perceived behavioral control and perceived barriers represented the externally controlled aspects. W. The model also showed the expected relationship between self-efficacy expectations (2) and behavioral intention (3). It was hypothesized that health care self-efficacy would be positively related to intention to participate in health care and that intention would predict participation behavior. WW. As shown in Figure 4, it was anticipated that the decision support intervention ( 1) would significantly increase health care self-efficacy expectations (2) and that increased self-efficacy would result in increased intention to participate in health care (3). Intention to participate was hypothesized to predict self-report of actual participation in health care (4) which would result in increased satisfaction with the health care decision (5). W. In the health care self-efficacy model, satisfaction with decision (5) was the desired outcome stemming from patient participation behavior. It was hypothesized that self-reported participation in health care (4) would be significantly associated with increased satisfaction with health care decision (5). Hypothesis 2 Statemmmfiflmolhosia Outcome Expectations, Subjective Norms and Health Care Self-Efficacy will significantly predict Intention to Participate in health care decisions. SummanLoLRelatedliteramre According to the theory of planned behavior, behavioral intention was determined by three factors, Attitude Toward a Behavior, Subjective Norm and Perceived Behavioral Control (Shifter 8: Ajzen, 1985). Attitude Toward a Behavior had two determinants, behavioral beliefs and outcome evaluation. 43 Behavioral beliefs was defined as the likelihood that performing a behavior would result in a given outcome and outcome evaluation was defined as the positive or negative value placed on a given outcome (Ajzen 8: Fishbein, 1980). These two concepts were identical to the concepts Outcome Expectation (Bandura, 1977) and Outcome Value (Teasdale, 1978) in self-efficacy theory (Tedesco, Keffer 8: Fleck-Kandath, 1991). Wong. Outcome expectations were defined as the belief that a given behavior would lead to a specific outcome (Bandura, 1977). According to the Theory of Planned Behavior, the determinants of Attitude Toward the Behavior were behavioral beliefs (subjective probability that performing the behavior will lead to a given outcome) and the outcome’s subjective value (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Each behavioral belief and outcome value were multiplied together and the products summed to produce Attitude Toward the Behavior (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Behavioral beliefs were identical to Bandura’s (1977) Outcome Expectations. Teasdale (1978) clarified the nature of outcome expectation as the sum of the probability of each outcome times the value of that outcome. Solomon and Annis (1990) used Teasdale’s (1978) and Ajzen 8: Madden’s (1986) method of multiplying outcome expectations by their value when developing a measure of outcome expectancy related to drinking behavior. Although outcome expectations, alone or in combination with self-efficacy expectations, have not been found to predict behavior as well as self-efficacy expectations alone (Solomon 8: Annis, 1990; Manning 8: Wright, 1983); they have been found to contribute significantly to prediction of behavioral intention (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986; Manstead, Proffitt 8: Smart, 1983; Maddux, Sherer 8: Rogers, 1982; Crawley, 1990; Netemeyer 8: Burton, 1990; Shifter 8: Ajzen, 71985)‘ Maddux, Sherer and Rogers (1982) found that outcome expectancy was significantly related to behavioral intention to use a novel communication 44 technique and that outcome expectancy was correlated with self-efficacy. thjggfixeflonn. Subjective norm had two determinants; normative beliefs and strength of normative beliefs. A normative belief was the perceived likelihood that other people would approve or disapprove of a given behavior. Subjective norm consisted of the product of each normative belief and its strength (motivation to comply with the other’s belief) (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Subjective norm has been found to significantly contribute to prediction of behavioral intention for some behaviors but not for others (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986; Beale 8: Manstead, 1991 ; Brubaker 8: Wickersham, 1990;McCau1, O’Neill 8: Glasgow, 1988). W. Attitude toward behavior, subjective norm and self-efficacy have been found to significantly predict dental hygiene behavior intentions (R2 = .31) (McCaul, O’Neill 8: Glasgow, 1988), intention to practice testicular self-examination (Brubaker 8: Wickersham, 1990), intention to exercise (Gatch 8: Kendzierski, 1990), intention to lose weight (Shifter 8: Ajzen, 1985), and intention to attend college class lectures (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). ‘ RolafionfoModol Hypothesis 2 regarded the prediction of intention to participate in health care (3) from the factors health care self-efficacy (2), outcome expectations (8) and subjective norm (9). As shown in the model of patient participation behavior, intention to participate was hypothesized to be determined by self-efficacy expectations, outcome expectations and subjective norms. It was hypothesized that all three factors would contribute significantly to prediction of intention. The participant modeling exercise in intervention C (1) targeted self-efficacy (2) as shown in the model, but was not part of the model itself. The model described the hypothesized relationships among self-efficacy, outcome expectations, 45 subjective norms, perceived barriers and intention, behavior and satisfaction. Hypothesis 3 W Health Care Self-Efficacy and Intention to Participate will significantly predict Participation in Health Care. WWW Self-efficacy represented the internal control aspect of perceived behavioral control (Shifter 8: Ajzen, 1985) and has been found to significantly improve prediction of behavior beyond behavioral intention (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986; Shifter 8: Ajzen, 1985; Tedesco, Keffer 8: Fleck-Kandath, 1991). Although self- efficacy may influence behavior through its impact on behavioral intention, it has been found to be more highly correlated with some behaviors than behavioral intention suggesting a direct, unmediated relationship to behavior as well. For example, self-efficacy was found to correlate .41 with exercise behavior, while intention was correlated .32 (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990). The degree to which self-efficacy (perceived behavioral control) has a direct influence on behavior has been hypothesized to depend on the degree of actual control present in a given situation. Perceived behavioral control has been found to improve prediction of behavior in situations in which individuals lack complete behavioral control and have experience with the behavior in question (Beale 8: Manstead, 1991 ; Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Since active participation in health care has been found to be a situation in which some individuals perceive a lack of internal control (self-efficacy) (Woodward 8: Wallston, 1987), it was anticipated that perceived behavioral control would exert a direct influence on participation behavior as well as impact intention to participate. According to the theory of planned behavior, perceived control represents actual behavioral control and experience results in a more accurate prediction of actual control (Ajzen 8: 46 Madden, 1986). Since the health care encounter was a situation with which most midlife women had experience, it was expected that their prediction of actual control would be highly accurate and therefore perceived control would be representative of the actual control available in that situation and directly related to behavior. WM As shown in Figure 4, it was hypothesized that there would be a direct relationship between self-efficacy (2) and participation behavior (4) not accounted for by behavioral intention (3) alone. It was therefore hypothesized that intention and self-efficacy together would significantly predict participation in health care decisions (4). Intervention C was hypothesized to exert an influence on behavior only via self-efficacy. Hypothesis 4 Siafcmentofflmofliesis Self-Efficacy Expectations and Outcome Expectations will be significantly correlated. SummancofRelamiLm According to theory, attitude toward the behavior and perceived behavioral control would be expected to be correlated (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986) however, some studies have found a correlation between the two factors (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990), while others have not (Shifter 8: Ajzen, 1985). Social cognitive theory maintained that while self-efficacy expectations and outcome expectations were distinct constructs, self-efficacy expectations and outcome expectations would be correlated except when no level of competence could produce desired outcomes (Bandura, 1988). Self-efficacy has been found to be significantly correlated with outcome expectations in several studies (Manning 8: Wright, 1983; Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990) supporting the theory. It has 47 also been shown that self-efficacy expectations and outcome expectations can be manipulated independently (Davis 8: Yates, 1982) and that outcome expectations contributed separately to self-efficacy’s prediction of behavior (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990; Manning 8: Wright, 1983) supporting the distinction between the factors. R ' t l The patient participation model (figure 4) paralleled the theory of planned behavior and health care self-efficacy (2) was therefore expected to be correlated with outcome expectations (8) since it was anticipated that patients would perceive a relationship (positive or negative) between patient participation behavior and quality of medical care. Intervention C was expected to influence self-efficacy expectations directly. Hypothesis 5 Siatemontotflxoothoaii Perceived Barriers and Health Care Self-Efficacy will contribute significantly to prediction of Intention to Participate in Health Care Decisions and participation behavior. SummancoLRelatedLiteramre According to the theory of planned behavior, perceived behavioral control included the elements of internal, personal control (skills and abilities) and control by external sources (time, opportunity, and other people) (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). The internal control factor was conceptually identical to self- efficacy (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986; Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990; Catch 8: Kendzierski, 1990; Brubaker 8: Wickersham, 1990) and the external control factor was similar to the concept ”perceived barriers” in the Health Belief Model (Beale 8: Manstead, 1991). Combined, self-efficacy and perceived barriers included both the internal and external control aspects of Perceived Behavioral Control. 48 In the Theory of Planned Behavior, both internal and external aspects of perceived behavioral control contributed to prediction of intention and behavior. According to social cognitive theory, perceived self-efficacy was the determining factor in intention and behavior: ”Human behavior is, of course governed largely by perceptions Of personal efficacy and social environments rather than simply by their Objective properties. Thus, individuals who believe themselves to be inefficacious are likely to effect limited change even in environments that provide many opportunities. Conversely, those who have a firm belief in their efficacy, through ingenuity and perseverance, figure out ways of exercising some measure of control in environments containing limited opportunities and many constraints,” (Bandura 8: Wood, 1989; p. 806). Recent research has suggested that self-efficacy alone was a better predictor of intention and behavior than perceived behavioral control which incorporated both internal and external control factors. Dzewaltowski, Noble and Shaw (1990) found that self-efficacy was more highly correlated with behavioral intention (r = .81) and exercise behavior (r = .41) than was perceived behavioral control (r = .40 and .10, respectively). Perceived behavioral control has however, been found to significantly contribute to prediction of behavioral intention and behavior in several studies (Dzewaltowski, Noble 8: Shaw, 1990; Beale 8: Manstead, 1991 ; Gatch 8: Kendzierski, 1990; Netemeyer 8: Burton, 1990; Shifter 8: Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). According to the theory of planned behavior, perceived control predicted behavior because it was a substitute for a measure of actual control (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986). Since it was actual behavioral control which was believed to predict behavior in the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 8: Madden, 1986), external control factors may add to prediction of behavior. 49 BolationjoModol As shown in the model, it was hypothesized that perceived barriers to participation (6) would contribute significantly to prediction of intention to participate (3) and participation behavior (4). Intervention C was expected to directly influence self-efficacy (2) but not perceived barriers (6) since external, or system-level barriers were outside the realm of the intervention. Table 2 shows the relationship between the model, hypotheses, instruments and analyses. The numbers in parentheses refer to Figure 4. 50 1:1th protheees Moots mm: 1 ___axs_sAn 1 i 1. Intervention C (1) 0Health Care Self- 1 0Multiple Regression will increase self- Efficacy Scale 1 0Path Analysis efficacy (2) which OBehavioral will influence Intentions Scale Intention (3) 8: 0Behavior Self- Participation (4) Report Scale which will impact OSatisfaction with Satisfaction (5) Decision Scale 2. Outcome OOutcome OPath Analysis Expectations (8), Expectations Scale Self-Efficacy 0Health Care Self- Expectations (2) 8: Efficacy Scale Subjective Norms OSubjective Norms (9) will predict Scale Intention (3) OBehavioral Intentions Scale 3. Self-Efficacy 0Health Care Self- OPath Analysis Expectations (1) 8: Efficacy Scale Intention (3) will OBehavioral predict ' Intentions Scale Participation OBehavior Self- Behavior (4) Report Scale 51 W Hmothegs Hypothesis Regiments Analgesic 4. Self-Efficacy (2) 8: 0Health Care Self- OPearson Correlation Outcome Efficacy Scale Coefficient Expectations (8) OOutcome will be Expectations Scale significantly correlated 5. Perceived Barriers OPerceived Barriers 0 Path Analysis (6) 8: Health Care Self-Efficacy (2) will predict Intention to Participate (3) and Participation Behavior (4) Scale 0Health Care Self- Efficacy Scale CBehavioral Intentions Scale OBehavior Self- Report Scale CHAPTER 2 Method ms The experimental interventions were conducted by the Decision Making in Menopause Study in classrooms at Michigan State University. Classrooms were equipped with desks and overhead projectors. ar P ' i Went. Three hundred seventy nine women were recruited by the Decision Making in Menopause Study through advertisements in the city newspaper, stories in community newspapers and radio and television announcements. Advertisements were directed at women over 40 who wanted to learn more about menopause and hormone replacement therapy. Most responses came from advertisements in the city newspaper. The newspaper in which the advertisements appeared had a circulation of 322,100 adults, 167,300 of whom were women. By age, the newspaper circulation was 90,700 for age 35—49; 51,200 for age 50—64; and 39,900 for age 65 and older. Since approximately 52% of the circulation was women, it was estimated that the advertisements potentially reached about 42,000 women between the ages of 45 and 64. The total population of women age 45 and over in the tri-county area was 60,348 in 1990. Interested persons were directed to call the study for information. See Appendix A for advertisement. Women who called were told that the purpose of the study was to test three educational programs to help women in their decision making related to menopause and hormone replacement therapy. They were informed that participation required attendance at up to three class sessions, completion of monthly calendars and attendance at follow-up sessions to complete questionnaires. Random assignment was described and explained as similar to a 52 53 lottery to ensure the women understood that they would have an equal chance of being in one of the two classroom groups or the brochure group. The three programs were described and participants who agreed to random assignment were registered for the study. W. Random assignment was accomplished by the author or assistants following a written protocol and using a table of random numbers. Registration forms were placed in alphabetical order or order of registration and a one-digit number was read from the table for each registration form. Participants were assigned to Group A if the number on the table was a 1, 2 or 3; Group B if the number was a 4, 5 or 6 and Group C if the number was a 7, 8 or 9. If the number read was a zero the next number in the column was used. Participants were informed of their group assignment at, not prior to, the first data collection session to prevent selective attrition based on group assignment. Am. Three hundred seventy nine (379) women registered for the study. Three hundred (300) women attended session 1. Of those who registered but failed to attend session I, approximately half canceled by phone prior to the program. The most frequent reasons given for cancellation were scheduling conflicts or illness. Of the women who attended session I, 252 (84%) completed all sessions and provided Time 1 and Time 2 data. Four participants were eliminated from analyses because they were not randomly assigned to group, resulting in a total sample size of 248 women at the conclusion of the intervention program. Distribution across experimental groups was as follows: Group A=87; Group B=78; Group C=83. Two hundred and two participants returned two-month follow-up questionnaires by mail (53% of those who registered; 67% of those who attended session I; 80% of those who attended session III). Distribution across experimental groups for the 2-month follow-up was: Group A=76; Group B=62; Group C=64. Of the 202 who completed the 54 follow-up questionnaires, 67 had visited their health care providers and provided behavior self-report data. Distribution across experimental groups was: Group A=22; Group B=24; Group C=21. Table 3 shows attrition. Attrition was monitored by comparing the demographic and other data from participants who terminated participation in the study with data from participants who continued in the study. Chi-square’s or Pearson correlation coefficients were computed comparing participants who dropped between session I, session III and follow-up. There were no differences in class membership, history of hysterectomy or oophorectomy, time since last menstrual period (menopausal status), education, or self-efficacy at time 1. There was selective attrition by experimental group, with more Group A participants than Group B or Group C participants continuing participation. It is hypothesized that this difference was due in part to less demand made on Group A participants. Group A participants needed only to attend sessions I and III to complete questionnaires, while Group B and Group C participants had to attend session II as well. Group B and Group C participants who attended sessions I and III but failed to attend the second session were counted as attrition since they did not receive the full treatment. Of those who remained in the study at session III, there was no difference in attrition by experimental group at follow-up. There was no significant attrition by experimental group between session III and follow-up. There was selective attrition by race, with more non-white participants dropping from the study between session I and session 111 than would be expected due to chance alone. The intervention may have been less useful for non-white women because of the small number participating. Attrition by race was constant across experimental groups and attrition between session 1]] and follow-up was not significantly different by race. 55 Table 3 A a r Group A Group B Group C Can. TOTAL Registered Wave I 51 52 53 156 Wave II 62 64 75 22 223 Total 113 116 128 22 379 Session I Wave I 41 (80%) 41 (79%) 37 (70%) 119 (76%) (0/0 Of 0 o o o Registr.) Wave II 55 (89 A.) 57 (89 A.) 69 (92 A.) 181 (81 A.) Total 96 (85%) 98 (84%) 106 (83%) 300 (79%) Session II Wave I 38 (93%) 33 (89%) 71 (91%) (‘70 0f 0 o 0 Session I) Wave II 50 (88 A.) 56 (81 A.) 106 (84 A.) Total 88 (90%) 89 (84%) 177 (87%) Session 11] Wave I 39 (95%) 34 (83%) 29 (78%) 102 (86%) (0/0 Of 0 o o 0 Session 1) Wave II 49 (89 A.) 47 (82 A.) 54 (78 A.) 150 (83 A.) Total 88 (92%) 81 (83%) 83 (78%) 252 (84%) Follow-up Wave I 33 (80%) 29 (71%) 26 (70%) 88 (74%) (2 mos '1 Wave II 43 (78%) 33 (58%) 38 (55%) 114 (63%) Total 76 (67%) 62 (53%) 64 (50%) 202 (53%) (% Reg) (% of (79%) (63%) (60%) (67%) Sess. 1) (% of (86%) (77%) (77%) (80%) Sess. 3) 56 Experience with menopausal symptoms affected attrition during the intervention programs. Participants who had never experienced symptoms were more likely to complete the intervention. Those who were not currently experiencing symptoms but had in the past were less likely to complete the intervention. There was no difference in return of follow-up questionnaires by experience with symptoms although participants who had never experienced menopausal symptoms were less likely to provide behavior self-report data (i.e. they were less likely to have visited a health care provider during the follow-up period). Difference in attrition during the intervention may have been due to the decreased relevance the intervention might have for women whose symptoms were in the past rather than current or anticipated. There was no difference in experience with menopausal symptoms at time 1 across experimental groups. In order to ensure adequate statistical power at follow-up (Time 2 and Time 3), twice as many participants were recruited at Time 1 as were needed at Time 3. See W515 below. WW. Participants were two hundred forty eight women who were at least 40 years old, who were participating in a study of menopause education programs. The women were recruited by the Michigan State University Decision Making in Menopause Study through media requests for participants (Rothert, 1990). Selected demographic characteristics of the sample and the Tri-County Area from which the sample was drawn are shown in Table 4. Comparable data for the Tri-County Area was not available for employment status. Sixty-three percent of the sample was employed full-time; 20% part-time, 5% were retired; 11% were not employed and 2% listed ”other" ' for employment. The sample was primarily White, employed full- or part-time, with incomes of at least $50,000 per year. Health history and access to care are shown in Table 5. Table 4 D m 1 . C1 . l' E S 1 Variable N % Tri-County Area Age 40-45 92 37.1% 46-50 114 46.0% 51-55 34 13.7% 56-60 6 2.4% 61-65 2 .8% Race (Females age 40—64) African-American 9 3.6% 5.3% Hispanic 4 1.6% (2.2%) American Indian 0 0.0% 0.8% White 234 94.40/0 91 .50/0 Asian / Pacific Islander 0 0.0% 1.4% Other 1 .4% 1.0% Household Income Under $14,999 10 4.0% 21.0% $15,000-$49,999 100 40.3% 53.0% $50,000-$99,999 113 45.6% 23.0% $100,000 and over 24 9.7% 3.0% Missing 1 .4% -- Education (Persons age 25+) Less than 12 years 2 .8% 16.0% High School Graduate 24 9.7% 28.0% Greater than 12 years but no degree 64 25.8% 24.0% Technical / Community College degree 31 12.5% 8.0% Bachelors Degree 59 23.8% 14.0% Graduate/ Professional Degree 63 25.4% 10.0% Other 4 1.6% -- Missing 1 .4% -- Marital Status (Females age 45+) Ever Married 231 93.1% 96.0% Never Married 17 6.9% 4.0% 58 Table 5 HeelthflstomandAoesaJsLMedisalLare variable N 0/0 Last Natural Menstrual Period Still Have Regular Periods 143 57.7% Less than 3 months ago 20 8.1% 3 to 12 Months ago 21 8.5% More than 12 months ago 59 23.8% Not Sure 5 2.0% Experience With Menopausal Symptoms Have Never Experienced Symptoms 43 17.3% Currently Experiencing Symptoms 123 49.6% Have Experienced Symptoms in the Past 22 8.9% Not Sure 60 24.2% Hysterectomy YES 36 14.50/0 N o 212 85.5% Oophorectomy Both Ovaries Removed 17 6.9% One Ovary Removed 8 3.2% No 223 89.9% History of Cancer Breast Cancer 4 1.6% Endometrial Cancer 4 1.6% Other Cancer 5 2.0% None 235 94.8% Source of Payment for Medications Completely Out-of-Pocket 31 12.5% Partly Out-of-Pocket/ Partly Other Source 187 75.4% Completely Paid by Other Source 30 12.1% 59 Beecerehllesigo This was a longitudinal experimental study with two experimental groups and a standard treatment control group. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three educational programs on menopause. Program A served as the control group with a written brochure as the standard intervention. Program B, 1 controlling for time and attention as well as information used a lecture and discussion format. Program C utilized an innovative decision support intervention developed by Rothert (1990) which consisted of exercises and role play to communicate the information. The independent variable was education method and the major dependent variables related to consumer participation in health care decisions were Self-efficacy; Perceived Barriers; Subjective Norm; Outcome Expectations; Behavioral Intention and Participation Behavior. Satisfaction with Decision was an outcome variable. Table 6 shows the schedule of administration for each measure. W Power was calculated for a two-tail F-test because although the major interest was in a positive relationship, a negative relationship between the intervention and self-efficacy was potentially possible. For an analysis of variance, 21 participants per group would produce a statistical power of .80 for a large effect magnitude of .40 (f = .40) at a2 = .05, requiring a final total N of 63 (Cohen, 1988). It was also determined that an N of 63 would provide a power of 80 or greater for a multiple regression analysis with fewer than 16 independent variables. Power was also calculated for a medium effect size. It was found that an N of 156 (53 per group) would be required to detect a medium effect size (f = .25) for analysis of variance and a multiple regression analysis with fewer than 13 independent variables. Power ([3) for detecting a small effect size (f = .02) with =248 was less than 29. See Appendix B. Health Care Self- Efficacy Scale 60 ul Iimel Health Care Self- Efficacy Scale Perceived Barriers Scale Subjective Norms and Motivation to Comply Instrument Outcome Expectations and Outcome Values Scale Intention to Participate in Health Care Decisions Item Iimel Health Care Self- Efficacy Scale Behavior Self-Report Instrument Satisfaction with Decision Scale 61 Proceflre 1413mm Program instructOrs were members of the Decision Making in Menopause Study research team. Two instructors team-taught each intervention session for programs B and C and attended the data collection sessions for program A. Instructors were organized into clinician / non-clinician teams. Each team member practiced the appropriate portion of the intervention in front of an audience prior to implementation of the intervention. Timing, content and delivery were tested in an intervention pilot (see below) which was attended by all instructors to ensure consistency across instructors. There was minor ’ disruption of instructor teams due to absenteeism. Absent members of an instructor team were replaced by a practiced instructor from another team. Clinicians were replaced by team members trained as clinicians and non- clinicians were replaced by non-clinicians. One member of the research team was trained in both clinician and non-clinician aspects of the intervention and served as substitute where possible. There were 2 instances of replaced instructors for Programs B or C out of 24 sessions. Table 7 shows the balancing of instructor team across group and wave. As expected, one-way Analysis of Variance failed to indicate differences on outcome variables by instructor team, supporting the assumption that the intervention was implemented consistently across instructor teams. Co- instructors served as manipulation checks for each other, assuring that each intervention session was implemented as designed. 62 Table 2 r h 1 Instructor Teams Experimental Group Wave Class A B C 1 Y Z X 1 2 Z X Y 3 X Y Z 2 4 Y Z X I | I' 2.! | Prior to implementation of the intervention study, outlines of the content and format for each session of Program B and Program C were developed. The handouts and intervention instruments for Program C (see Inelntemenflnn, below) were prepared and pilot-tested on a convenience sample of 21 women age 40 and over recruited from among the faculty and staff of a large rnidwestern university (see InstnnnenLBith section for information on recruitment). Participants were contacted by phone regarding participation in the intervention pilot. Those who agreed to attend all three sessions were registered. Pilot study participants received a buffet luncheon and a draft copy of the brochure developed for the intervention. The intervention pilot for Program C was held during the lunch hour for three consecutive days in a private dining room at a convenient campus location. All three intervention sessions were video recorded. The video recordings were transcribed and used in modifying the intervention outlines. Questions 63 generated by pilot participants during the question and answer period in the intervention were compiled and the answers were either incorporated into the intervention or anticipated in future question and answer periods. Answers were generated so that they would be consistent across instructors. Feedback from the intervention pilot indicated that participants liked the question and answer period and the informational content. Participants felt that adequate time for the program and clarity of the intervention instruments could ’ be improved. It was also suggested that audio-visual aids be used during the lecture portions of the program. Based on the intervention pilot, the program content was modified and reorganized. Some aspects of the intervention, such as inclusion of a bar chart showing women’s decisions related to hormone replacement therapy, were eliminated. Instead this information was conveyed briefly in a descriptive format based on feedback that the bar chart was confusing. Other content was reordered to improve understandability and use of time. Over-head transparencies were added to the lecture portions of the programs and the intervention instruments were modified for understandability. Ihelntemenfion mnenjmehmelntemenfien. The written brochure, described in the section W provided information on menopause, hormone replacement therapy and communication with health care professionals (Rothert, et al., 1992). The brochure comprised the entire intervention for participants in Program A, and provided a written adjunct to the lectures for Programs B and C. See Appendix C for outline. W. The second program (Program B) consisted of three 1 1/ 2 hour sessions usinga lecture format with overhead transparencies as visual aids. Participants in Program B received copies of the brochure to use 64 during the lectures which paralleled the brochure in content. In addition, participants were provided with 3X5 cards on which they could write questions to be read and answered by the instructor during the group question and answer period. To help participants think about the HRT decision, data was presented which depicted how different women may weigh various factors related to the decision. This helped to introduce discussion of personal risk factors and to show that women vary in their values related to those factors. Information about the impact of personal risk factors on heart disease, osteoporosis and endometrial cancer was presented and participants were encouraged to think about their personal values related to each of the factors important to the HRT decision. The format and content of Program B was discussed in detail in the sectionlhe W. See Appendix C for an outline of the content and format of the three sessions. W. The third program (Program C) was a decision support intervention developed to aid women in decision making related to their menopausal health. See Appendix C for outline. The first session of the program was identical in content to Programs A and B. Session I of Intervention C was identical in format as well as content to Session I of Program B. For the initial wave of participants, the lecture on the pros and cons of HRT was also included in Session 1. Time constraints resulted in modification of the program so that this information was presented in Session H for Wave II programs. As in Program B, a question and answer period using 3X5 cards was included following presentation of the pros and cons of hormone replacement therapy. As in Program B, data was presented which depicted how different women may weigh various factors related to the decision. The women then completed the Personal Risk Assessment, Problem Significance Assessment and 65 Relevance Chart. As the women marked risk on their relevance charts, they were first instructed to mark one box for base rate risk for each of the factors to help them understand the relative risk for each factor. For example, since heart disease is much more common than endometrial cancer, the heart disease risk ”arm” of the relevance chart was marked comparatively longer than the endometrial cancer risk ”arm.” Women then marked a specified additional number of boxes for their personal risk if it was above population base rate. Women who had few additional risk factors did not mark any additional boxes on the chart beyond that for base rate risk. The purpose of the relevance chart was to help women visually compare their risks and values across the multiple factors in their decision. Following the Personal Risk Assessment exercise, the women completed the Problem Significance Assessment. The women scored their Problem Significance Assessments in the following way: A factor rated 30 or below was of low significance, a factor rated greater than 30 but less than 70 was of moderate significance and a factor rated 70 or greater was of high significance. This information was transferred in graphic form to the Relevance Chart. The three factors for which risk information was relevant (heart disease, osteoporosis and endometrial cancer) had two ”arms” on the relevance chart; one for risk and one for significance. For these factors the women marked one box on the significance arm for factors they rated as low significance, two boxes for factors moderate in significance and all three boxes for factors rated high in significance for their decision making. For the other factors only the significance arm was present and it was marked in the same way. After completion of the Personal Risk Assessment and Problem Significance Assessment, the women participated in the scenarios exercise to help them think about how to combine their personal risks, values and base rate risk. 66 Following the scenarios exercise, the women completed the Relevance Chart and made a decision about whether to consult their health care providers about taking HRT. Session III included the lecture and role play on communicating with health professionals. See the section, W for a detailed description of the intervention. Women in all three intervention programs completed a personal plan form during week 3 of the intervention. The women chose up to three new activities to prevent disease, promote health or manage symptoms. The women were asked to choose activities in one of four categories; 1) exercise, 2) calcium intake, 3) HRT or 4) other. They recorded their planned activities on the personal plan form along with how often they planned to engage in the activity. The women were asked to keep a monthly calendar indicating when they engaged in each planned activity. 9 II 'I' E |° To decrease the likelihood of attrition from the study, a raffle for cash prizes was offered to participants in each of the three experimental groups. Participants in Programs B and C were expected to return for the Time 2 data collection because it was incorporated into the third session of their intervention programs. Therefore, participants in Programs B and C were only offered a raffle at the follow-up data collections (6 and 12 months post-intervention for the main study) and eligibility for the raffle was dependent on having returned all interim data. Participants in Program A (the brochure only group) were expected to be less likely to return at Time 2 since they had already received the complete intervention (the brochure) at Time 1. Therefore, participants in Program A were told that they would be eligible to participate in a raffle for one of three cash prizes ($25, $25, or $50) when they returned to complete questionnaires at Time 2. Participants in Program A also received a reminder post-card to attend the 67 Time 2 data collection since there was a one-week gap between Time 1 and Time 2 for this group. Participants in Programs B and C attended intervention sessions weekly decreasing the likelihood that they would forget to attend the third session of the program. Participants received monthly mailings from the study which was expected to help minimize attrition. Participants who failed to return follow-up questionnaires received at least one reminder telephone call. Participants who indicated that they had discarded the questionnaires (usually through misunderstanding) were mailed replacement instruments. At least three attempts were made to reach participants by phone if they had not returned their questionnaires. Datamation Prior to commencement of the intervention program, participants completed written consent forms, results request forms and a series of written questionnaires. Data collection took approximately 45 minutes during sessions I and III of the intervention program period. Following data collection, participants were informed regarding whether they had been randomly assigned to the brochure-only program or one of the class programs. Participants in program A then received brochures and were free to return home. Participants in programs B and C received brochures and the Session I lecture. Participants were informed that participation in the study was completely voluntary and that they could terminate participation at any time without penalty. The instructor team answered questions about the study and what would be expected of the women during the study period. Participants in program A were informed that they would be able to participant in a raffle for one of three cash prizes when they attended the follow-up data collection at Time 2 (see Won). Prior to data collection, the purpose of the study and instructions for completing the 68 instruments were briefly presented. The Health Care Self-Efficacy Scale was completed at Time 1 and Time 2. Other instruments completed at Time 2 included the Perceived Barriers Measure, Subjective Norms Scale, Motivation to Comply scale, Outcome Expectations Scale, Outcome Values scale and Intention to Participate scale. Participants answered the questionnaires on a computer- scored answer sheet. Two months following the intervention, participants were mailed the follow- up instruments and instructed to complete and return them. The instruments included the Health Care Self-Efficacy scale, Behavior Self-Report Measure and Satisfaction with Decision Scale. Participants answered directly on the questionnaires rather than on a computer-scored answer sheet. Measms Instrumentmot All measurement instruments were pilot tested using a convenience sample of 120 women age 40 and over recruited from among the faculty and staff of a large rnidwestem university. A letter explaining the study was sent to all female faculty and academic staff (n=approximately 1,300) and non-academic staff age 40 and over (n=1,611). Two hundred fifteen women interested in more information returned a registration form by mail or called the study office. Of these, 185 were scheduled to attend one of the data collection sessions. Thirty women indicated interest in participating but were unable to attend any of the sessions and 54 women were excluded because the sessions they registered for were canceled. Several women canceled prior to the data collection session. The remaining women were scheduled to attend a session to complete questionnaires with 120 completing instruments. Instrument pilot participants received refreshments and a draft copy of the brochure developed for the intervention. Instrument pilot participants completed a consent form and draft copies of the 69 instruments and provided written and verbal feedback regarding the clarity of the instructions. The feasibility of using computer-scored answer sheets was also tested during the instrument pilot. All scales were found to have adequate internal consistency (a _>_ .70) and computer-scored answer sheets were found to be feasible with this population. Most participant comments were related to clarity of items and instructions. Confusing items and instructions were revised for clarity. Wm Healm Care $lf-Effieaey. A modified version of a measure developed by Woodward 8: Wallston (1987) was used (Woodward, 1984). The modified scale was pilot-tested for clarity and found to have adequate internal consistency (a = .91; M =63.82; SD = 13.12). The scale (Appendix D) consisted of 5 items measuring self-efficacy related to desire for control over health care and 3 items measuring self-efficacy related to desire for health information. The modified scale correlated .66 with the original scale. Responses were on a scale from 1 (Not At All Confident) to 10 (Extremely Confident). Responses were coded from 1 to 10. The scale score was calculated by taking the mean of all non-missing items on the scale resulting in a scale score with a range of 1 to 10. The Health Care Self-Efficacy scale was confirmed to have adequate internal consistency with the study sample at Time 1 (or = .91) and Time 2 (a = .93). The scale means and standard deviations at Time 1, Time 2 and Time 3 are shown in Appendix E. Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 7 show the frequency distributions for Health Care Self-Efficacy at Time 1, Time 2 and Time 3. A higher scale score indicated greater confidence in ability to participate actively in health care decisions. 7O l-Effia Tim 1Pr intrvn'n Di n 1|] 0 4 35—: __.___.4|—JA< O 5 3 2 a» d — H a) d 0 5 2 1 285$on ._._ mg mg new new was t mg .m. moo m mN.© NJ mum coo m~.m a mi. m as am mom 4... mum & m2 m~.~ m5 m3 71 O O .59! I.',,- ..._!l‘ ‘nt '1"- 0 5 5 4 0 4. 5 3 0 5 0 3 2 2 55:35 Ra Ra m3 m3 max t w A. mus M £8 cows mg m m2. m mg m m2 m mom on m2 m3. m5 m2 72 _U01-_L _ -11 ° m D. n I mum mud m3 mg m? t mg m. mks We mN.© NJ mum coo m mad a man. m . 0 mm a & m5. m m3 ax m2 mum m5 m3 5:25on 73 W. The Perceived Barriers scale (AppendixD) was a 5—item scale which assessed the impact of potential barriers to participation in decisions such as health care practitioner, payment options, health state, lack of access to information and conflicting information. Responses were on a 5- point scale with 3 anchors: 1 = Not at All, 3 = Somewhat and 5 = A Great Deal. The measure also included 2 open-ended items to assess other perceived barriers. Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .62. Appendix E shows the scale mean and standard deviation. A higher scale score indicated greater perceived barriers to participation in health care decisions. Table 8 shows the corrected item-total correlation for the scale items. Figure 8 shows the scale frequency distribution. Table 9 shows the frequency of responses to two open-ended items asking about additional barriers to participation. Responses to the two items over-lapped and were combined in the table. In cases where a participant wrote in the same answer for both questions, only one was counted. Where multiple barriers were mentioned by a single respondent, all were counted and listed under the heading ’mentioned.’ The heading ’N’ indicates the number of participants who listed a barrier in that category. Item Corrected Item-Total Correl How much does your doctor (or regular health care practitioner) limit your participation in medical decisions? .22 How much do your payment Options (such as health insurance) limit your participation in medical decisions? .29 How much does your health state or personal risk factors limit your participation in medical decisions? .38 How much does lack of access to accurate information limit your participation in medical decisions? .57 How much does conflicting medical advice limit your participation in medical decisions? .45 75 Fi 1 Di tributi nBarri r t Parti i ai n Fr 76 Table 2 L “1.!" 0 ‘ 101‘. 0 '10- - (1‘0 .1 ‘ Category Mentioned HealthLareEroxidmu 0Hard to find good provider 0Hard to get appointment with provider OProvider too busy / in a hurry GProvider doesn't listen OProvider arrogance/ personality OProvider doesn't inform patient OProvider discourages patient participation OGeneral distrust of health care providers 0Poor communication among multiple providers / fragmented care 33 0Limitations on provider choices 0Limitations on treatment choices OLimitations imposed by HMO's 0Lack of insurance/ cost OGeneral cost 18 16 E 11' 'l |' 0Personal lack of time OPersonal lack of knowledge OPersonal health problems 0Lack of personal support OFear/ Embarrassment/ Problem Making Decisions 26 OMedicine doesn't have all the answers 0Lack of research 0Conflicting advice 12 11 System-Rem OSystem not prevention-oriented 0Lack of sexual equality 0Lack of access to medical records OReferral system Miseellanmua OMakes own decisions 0Doesn't want to participate OFamily input OJOb OUnspecified 18 18 77 W. The Outcome Expectations scale was a 7-item measure which assessed the extent to which the individual expected that active patient participation behavior would lead to specific outcomes. An example item was, ”Patient participation in medical decisions and choices results in: the best medical care.” The response scale was a 5 point Likert-type scale with 1 = Extremely Unlikely and 5 = Extremely Likely with a midpoint of 3 = Neither Likely Nor Unlikely. For each outcome in the scale above, the respondent indicated its value on a 5 point scale, with 1=Extremely Bad and 5 = Extremely Good with a midpoint of 3 = Neither Bad Nor Good. The Outcome Expectations scale was computed by multiplying each expected outcome by its value and taking the mean of the non-missing values. The scale was found to have adequate internal consistency (a = .87). A higher scale score reflected an expectation that patient participation in decisions would result in the better medical care. A lower scale score reflected an expectation that patient participation would result in worse medical care. Table 10 shows the corrected item-total correlation for the scale items. The corrected item-total correlations shown are for the product of the expectation and value. Figure 9 shows the scale frequency distribution. See Appendix D for the instrument. Appendix E shows the scale mean and standard deviation. 78 - .qdq qqqd qd-q «an. dq-q d-<- _ _ _ _ H — 0 5 0 5 0 2 1 1 Gama—moi 79 .4- 03‘, 9‘ 1.11., 11‘ I" "Wu-1.0.. 01‘4”! Item Corrected Item-Total Correlatibn Patient participation in medical decisions and choices results in: 0Medical procedures that are unnecessary for the patient .55 0Medical procedures that are harmful for the patient .56 0The wrong medications for the patient .56 0The right medical tests and examinations for the patient .67 0The right treatment for the patient .79 0The right medical decisions for the patient .79 0The best medical care .72 W. The Subjective Norms measure was developed to assess the perceived opinion of others about patient participation in health care decisions. The items paralleled the desire to participate item developed by Strull, et al. (1984). The response scale was 1 = Clinician, 2 = Clinician after considering patient’s opinion, 3 = clinician and patient together equally, 4 = patient after considering clinician’s opinion, 5 = patient. The instructions for the scale were, ”Some people think that the decision to take hormone replacement therapy should be made by the clinician while other people think the decision should be made by the patient. Indicate how each of the following people think the decision should be made: Medical practitioner, spouse, relatives, friends, other (open-ended)” For each of the people listed, the individual answered the question, ”how much do you want to do what [this person] wants you to?” The 80 5-point response scale had anchors at 1 = Not At All and 5 = Very Much. For each item, the perceived norm was multiplied by the motivation to comply and the scale was computed by taking the mean of the non-missing items. Since each score was weighted by desire to comply, a higher scale score indicated a belief that significant others thought medical decisions should be made with greater patient participation and that there was high motivation to comply with their opinions. A lower score indicated a belief that others thought decisions should be made with less patient participation and that there was lower motivation to comply. See Appendix D for the measure. Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of internal consistency was .75. Appendix E shows the scale mean and standard deviation. Figure 10 shows the scale frequency distribution. Table 11 shows the corrected item-total correlation for the 4 scale items. The scale item indicated is the product of the subjective norm and motivation to comply. Table 12 shows responses to the open-ended item on the Subjective Norms measure. The table lists significant others who influenced the decision regarding whether or not to take hormone replacement therapy. 81 Item Corrected Item-Total Correlat_ibn Your doctor (or regular health care practitioner) thinks the the decision should be made by: .31 Your spouse, partner or significant other thinks the decision should be made by: .63 Your parents, relatives and children think the decision should be made by: .71 Your friends, peers and classmates think the decision should be made by: .53 mmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmm H H [\ H Normssweeasezesaeeaaaaa Scale Score Category Midpoint Table 12 10‘ aV‘. \0 1-1 10- ‘ f 82 n ‘ n D i ion he‘ll-En- ‘0 .1 Source N Meia 0Media in general OFeminist authors 0 Health writers OAuthors in general Self. RespeflecLthmgei OReligion 0Counselor 0 Parents Mmenflholflefixoefimee. OFriends OMother 0 Sisters OOther women in general IglowIedgeablehers. 0 Researchers ODoctor 02nd Opinion OFreinds / Relatives in Health Occupation IIS 't'i 83 Wm. This measure listed 8 behaviors related to the next visit to the health care provider (or = .85). The individual was asked to indicate the extent to which she intended to do each of the activities. The 5 point response scale had anchors at 1 = Not At All and 5 = Completely. The scale was computed by taking the mean of the non-missing items. See Appendix D for the measure. Appendix B shows the scale mean and standard deviation. A higher scale score indicated greater intention to participate actively in health care decisions. Figure 11 shows the scale frequency distribution. Table 13 shows the corrected item-total correlation for the 8 scale items. wad. mod wmfi mad 8 mmd wmd mfim ‘3. 6'8 N N ore Category Midpoint co cq N SC e H 28% we.“ m3 ,.wme ..meH 120 - 1001 _ 0 8 4 1 E — a w 55853.5 85 Table 13 311.. '0 -. .1 ‘19.: lo. ‘ 01‘ ‘9 ‘ 1- 0 ._ o '11“ Item Corrected Item-Total Correlation Intention to: Carefully consider and understand my lanes related to the tradeoffs of hormone replacement therapy or other care .44 Gather the information I need to make an informed decision about hormone replacement therapy or other care .57 Carefully consider my mm related to hormone replacement therapy or other care. .61 Ask for an extended appointment to discuss my concerns about my health care .52 Prepare for my visit by thinking about the questions I want to ask, smiteihemfinm and bring the list with me to the visit .67 At the beginningof the visit, tell my doctor / practitioner that I have questions I would like to discuss. .67 During my visit, ask the questions I had prepared, repeat the answers to be sure I understand them and aelsjm: clanfieahonineeessam .67 After my visit, review the visit, follow the treatments or recommendations agreed upon and call my doctor / practitioner if I have additional questions or unexpected side effects. .67 86 W. The behavior self-report measure included 6 items regarding participation behavior during the last visit to a medical practitioner (or = .86). The 5-point response scale had anchors at 1 = Not At All and 5 = Completely. Behaviors assessed included: considered values, personal risks and gathered information prior. to visit, asked questions during the visit, and followed recommendations and reported side effects after the visit. A final question was adapted from an item developed by Strull, et al. (1984). The item asked, ”Which Of the following choices best describes how the decision about whether or not you would take hormone replacement therapy mentally made?” Response choices ranged from 1) The clinician made the decision, using all that’s known about hormone replacement therapy to 5) I made the decision using all I know and learned about hormone replacement therapy. Response choices parallel the item on desire to participate in decisions. See Appendix D for the measure. Appendix E shows the scale mean and standard deviation. A higher scale score indicated more active participation in health care decisions. Figure 12 shows the scale frequency distribution. Table 14 shows the corrected item-total correlation for the 8 scale items. 87 I12 Di tri ' nB havi r lf-R al Fr Dame—moi 88 5‘ 1 .0 1‘1-',",001‘t1-1' .1. .. .1!--." . ‘I-Q. Item Corrected Item-Total Correlation Carefully considered and understood my 1311.128. related to the tradeoffs of hormone replacement therapy or other care. .71 Gathered the information I needed to make an informed decision about hormone replacement therapy or other care. .66 Carefully considered my nemnaLrislga related to hormone replacement therapy or other care. .51 Asked for an extended appointment to discuss my concerns about my health care. .41 Prepared for my visit by thinking about the questions I wanted to ask, W and bringing the list with me to the visit. .71 At themginnjng of the visit, told my doctor/ practitioner that I had questions I would like to discuss. .67 During my visit, asked the questions I had prepared, repeated the answers to be sure I understood them and askedfor elanficahonifnmessam. .75 After my visit, reviewed the visit, followed the treatments or recommendations agreed upon and called my doctor/ practitioner if I had additional questions or unexpected side effects. .65 89 Wen. This 6-item measure, developed by Rothert (1990) was based on the O’Connor 8: O’Brien-Pallais (1990) model of an effective decision. Example items included, ”The decision I made was the best decision possible for me personally,” and ” I am satisfied with my decision.” The S-point Likert-type response scale ranged from 1 = Strongly Disagree to 5 = Strongly Agree. The scale was computed by taking the mean of the non-missing items (a = .91). See Appendix D for measure. The Satisfaction with Decision scale was confirmed to have adequate internal consistency with the study sample at Time 3 (a = .91) Appendix B shows the scale mean and standard deviation. Figure 13 shows the frequency distribution for the scale at time 3. mlelntercorrelefions Table 15 shows the intercorrelations among the scales. The alpha coefficients of internal consistency are shown on the diagonal. Correlations corrected for unreliability are shown above the diagonal (Edwards, 1954). See Appendix F for calculation formula. Correlations which were significant at p < .05 are indicated by an asterisk. 90 uu-d «ddwqfifiqd—uuqq—uu- wmm kudoswfim 8.8 8.8 wow 2.... t wen .m. mod N wm.m NJ mam coo m wwd a no.“ m mom & an m we; & «.3 w? m: 91 1321.115. Carrelatien Manix Beh. Beh. Sat Self-E Outc. Subj. Barr. SE SE Int. /Dec. T2 Exp. Nrm T1 T3 Beh. .85 .44 .15 .17 .06 .07 -.04 .17 .13 Intent. (248) Beh. .38* .86 .50 .05 —.17 —.02 —.37 .12 .38 (67) (67) Sat. .13 .44 .91 .45 .22 .11 -.42 .38 .59 w/dec. (184) (67) (184) Self-Bf .15” .05 .41” .93 .35 —.06 —.60 .73 .74 T 2 (248) (67) (184) (248) Outcm .05 -.15 .20* .31* .87 .04 —.18 .21 .28 Expect. (248) (67) (184) (248) (248) Subj. .05 —.02 .09 —.05 .04 .75 .00 .03 .07 Norm (243) (66) (180) (243) (243) (243) Barr. —.03 -.27* —.32* —.45* -.13 .03 .62 —.43 —.62 (247) (67) (184) (247), (247) (242) (247) Self-Bf .15" .10 .34" .68" .19" .03 -.32* .91 .67 T 1 (244) (65) (182) (244) (244) (239) (243) (244) Self-Bf .12 .34" .54" .69" .26" .06 -.47* .61" .93 T3 (184) (67) (180) (184) (184) (180) (184) (182) (184) "p<.05 CHAPTER 3 Results The purpose of this research was to determine the impact of three menopause education programs on health care self-efficacy of midlife women and to test a model of the impact of self-efficacy on participation in health care decisions. Data on Self-Efficacy, Outcome Expectations, Subjective Norms, Intention to Participate and Participation Behavior were collected from 248 participants in a menopause education study and used to explore 5 hypotheses. The hypotheses regarded the effect of the interventions on self-efficacy and patient participation behavior related to medical decisions. Moses. Emmet Hypothesis 1 predicted that participants in the Decision Support Intervention (Program C) would have greater confidence in their ability to participate actively in health care decisions which would result in greater intention to participate, increased participation behavior and greater satisfaction with decision. Table 15 shows the correlations between self—efficacy (Time 1 and Time 2), perceived barriers, subjective norms, outcome expectations, behavioral intention and behavior self-report. A multiple regression analysis was conducted with Self-Efficacy at Time 2 as the dependent variable. Self-Efficacy at Time 1 was entered on the first step to control for preexisting between-participant differences. Experimental group was contrast-coded (Cohen 8: Cohen, 1983) and the two contrast-coded group variables were entered on the second step. Contrast 1 compared Experimental Group C (Decision Support Intervention) with Groups B (Lecture / Discussion) and A (Brochure Only). Contrast 2 Compared Group B with A. Table 16 shows 92 93 the coding scheme for experimental group. Two variables representing the interaction between Self-Efficacy at Time 1 and experimental group were entered on a third step. The nominal variable experimental group was contrast-coded orthogonally so that the sum of the products of the coding coefficients was zero. See Appendix G for computation. Table 17 shows the regression coefficients (B) and their standard errors (s.e.), standardized regression coefficients ([3) and their standard errors (s.e.), partial correlation coefficients (pri ), Multiple R, and R2- change for each step in the analysis. The constant represents the average Y intercept and is used to calculate the intercepts for each of the three regression equations. An asterisk indicates the F values which were statistically significant at p < .05. Self-Efficacy at Time 1 significantly predicted Self-Efficacy at Time 2 (Rz-change = .46, p < .05). The two contrast-coded variables representing the variance due to experimental group did not significantly increase prediction of Self-Efficacy at Time 2 (Rz-change = .01, p > .05). The interaction between experimental group and self-efficacy at Time 1 was carried by two variables (interaction 1 and interaction 2) computed from the products of Time 1 Self- Efficacy and the two contrast-coded group variables respectively. The interaction between Time 1 Self-Efficacy and Experimental Group contributed significantly to prediction of Self-Efficacy at Time 2 (R2-change = .02, p < .05). Figure 14 shows Self-Efficacy Post-Intervention (Time 2) plotted against Self-Efficacy Pre- Intervention (Time 1) for each experimental group. Table 18 shows the regression equations for Self-Efficacy post-intervention (Time 2). See appendix G for calculations. The hypothesis that participants in Program C would have greater confidence in ability to participate actively in health care decisions than groups A and B was not supported (Bcontmt 1 = .15; p > .05). There was however, a significant interaction effect between experimental group and self-efficacy at Time 1. 94 The experimental treatment differentially impacted those for whom self- efficacy was low pre-intervention compared to those for whom self-efficacy was high pre-intervention (Rz-chg = .02; p < .05). The significant difference was due to the contrast between Experimental Group B and Group A (B -.13; p < .05). interaction 2 = One-way Analysis Of Variance indicated that Behavioral Intention, Behavior Self-Report and Satisfaction with Decision were no greater for Experimental Group C than for Groups A and B. (F = .39, .04, .10, respectively; p > .05). Table 19 shows the means and standard deviations of each variable by experimental group. 121219.16. C C 1. ill . 1E . 15 V .1] Contrast Contrast Experimental Group Variable Variable 1 2 Brochure (A) —.5 -1 Lecture/ Discussion (B) —.5 +1 Decision Support (C) +1 0 Total 0 0 (-.5)(-1) + (--5)(1) + (1)(0) = 0 95 mo. v 8.. ................................................................................ .IOUUICIIIIIOIIIIII..0...- OIUIOO 0.0... COCO-II. ’0‘. ll... .ICOII IIOICIOOIII: ............................................... IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII .................. *Ow WNH m:.v mh.m AnguchUv 2.. $3 em.) 63 2... 268883 m m. 3.- a3 «7 338.1 H 88885 a N m~nw€m> .2. me an. .2. 63 N6. 83 NS 868:8 m H Giants! u.u.”.”.n.umwn ”Hung “mm” mm ”unwwuflwflnw “mnwwuw”HHHHNNMNNHWH ”w””Huuuuuwwmwmmw 2. .NC. 30¢ no. Amvo mm. ummbfloU N H ugh. .388 6+. .638 me. we. .63.: mo. 63 me. 33%. 889328 H mew... We as: “w... m m E 3.8 a 3.8 m 6363, new . H. . t u N . “4,. .. . .: - 41:1-.. .3. ~. .EE..xmw. .M .m H... . . .1. 3.1“. 1:-.. 3.... .4. .. . H W 96 W1 lf- ffi a T lf- f'c x ' ntal r Self-Efficacy Post-Intervention (Time 2) 10 Decision Support (C) Lecture / Discussion (B) / Brochure Only (A) I l I l r I l I l 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Self-Efficacy Pre-Intervention (Time 1) 97 31213.18. {'14: In a. 1!. 0, 1° -E' on ,0 ,.:“o -41: For Brochure Only Experimental Group (A): YA = .715(SET1) + 2.595 A For Lecture/ Discussion Experimental Group (B): YB = .455(SET1) + 4.835 A For Decision Support Experimental Group (C): YC = .540(SET1) + 3.940 Experimental Group Mean (s.d.) . (n) Vanable Brochure Lecture / Discuss Decision TOTAL (A) (B) Suvport (C) . 4.4 (.64) 4.5 (.63) 4.4 (.55) 4.4 (.60) “mm“ (87) (78) (83) (248) . 3.8 (.97) 3.7 (.65) 3.8 (.87) 3.8 (.83) Bahama (22) (24) (21) (67) . . 4.0 (.72) 4.0 (.62) 4.1 (.57) 4.1 (.64) sansfam‘m (68) (54) (60) (182) 98 HypothesesLiandfi Hypotheses 2, 3 and 5 related to testing the model of active patient participation behavior shown in Figure 4. Hypothesis 2 stated that Self-Efficacy, Outcome Expectations and Subjective Norms would significantly predict Intention to Participate in health care decisions. Hypothesis 3 stated that Health Care Self-Efficacy and Intention to Participate would significantly predict Participation Behavior. Hypothesis 5 stated that Perceived Barriers and Health Care Self-Efficacy would contribute significantly to prediction of Intention to Participate in Health Care Decisions and Participation Behavior. A path analysis using LISREL VII in SPSSX was conducted to analyze the extent to which the observed relationships among the variables fit the theoretical model. The intercorrelations among the variables were corrected for attenuation due to unreliability of the measures (See Appendix F for calculations) and the corrected correlation matrix was analyzed by LISREL VII. Table 15 shows the correlations among the variables analyzed. Missing data were handled by pairwise deletion. Figure 15 shows the path coefficients for the model as proposed. An asterisk indicates the paths which were significant. x2 with 7 degrees of freedom was 32.71 (p < .000) indicating a poor fit. Goodness of fit index was .90, adjusted goodness of fit was .58 and root mean square residual was .171. Significant residuals (indicating needed paths) were 3.74 between Self- Efficacy and Satisfaction with Decision and —2.16 between Barriers and Satisfaction with Decision. Table 20 shows the t-values for the paths in the model as proposed. 99 Outcome Expectations (x2) d» g. Subjective Norms (x3) (:5? d3 Satisfaction . Behavior (y2) _> with gapision 3 Behavioral Intention (y1) Self-Efficacy (x1) 714 = '05 Barriers (x4) *p < .05 100 181116.11 Model as Erbpbsed: t—valuee Intent Behav. Sat Self-Eff Outcm. Subj. Barr. w / Dec. T2 Expect. Norm Intent. 0.000 0.000 0.000 1.344 —0.030 0.655 .369 Behav. 4455* 0.000 0.000 -2.066* 0.000 0.000 -4.014* Sat 0.000 4.509” 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 w / Dec Based on the standardized residuals, t-values and modification indices the model was modified for better prediction of behavior and outcome. Figure 16 shows the path coefficients for the revised model. x2 with 10 degrees of freedom was 14.63 (p = .146) indicating a good fit. Goodness of fit index was .945, adjusted goodness of fit was .845 and root mean square residual was .107. No non- significant paths were included in the model. Table 21 shows the standardized residuals for the revised model. Table 22 shows the t-values fOr the revised model. Hypothesis 2 was not supported. Intention to Participate in Health Care Decisions was not predicted by Outcome expectations, Subjective Norms and Self-Efficacy. Although Self-Efficacy was significantly correlated with Intention 101 Egocolo 'i eri M 1 Outcome Expectations (x2) Behavioral Intention (y1) r = .31" Barriers (x4) Self-Efficacy (x1) *p < .05 Satisfaction with Decision (y3) 102 (SeeTable 15), it was not a significant predictor in the multivariate path analysis. Neither Outcome Expectations nor Subjective Norms were significantly correlated with Intention. Hypothesis 3 was partially supported. As shown in Figure 16, Behavioral Intention significantly predicted Participation Behavior, but Self-Efficacy did not. Self-Efficacy was not significantly correlated with Behavior (See Table 15). Hypothesis 5 was partially supported. Although neither Self-Efficacy nor Barriers significantly predicted Behavioral Intention, Barriers was a significant predictor of Participation Behavior (see Figure 16). Self-Efficacy was not a significant predictor of Participation Behavior. It was expected that Self-Efficacy and Barriers would be highly correlated and have similar patterns of correlation with other variables because they were believed to be opposite poles of the same construct, Perceived Behavioral Control (after Azjen 8: Madden, 1986). This assumption was found to be unsupported. Although Self-Efficacy and Barriers were moderately correlated (r = -.55; corrected for attenuation), Barriers but not Self-Efficacy was significantly correlated with Behavior (r = -.38 and .05 respectively—corrected for attenuation). See Appendix F for calculation of corrected correlation coefficient. According to the proposed model, outcome of Active Patient Behavior was satisfaction with the health care decision. The Path analysis indicated that Active Patient Behavior significantly predicted Satisfaction with Decision (see Figure 16). Unexpectedly, Self-Efficacy was also a predictor of Satisfaction with Decision (‘Y =.43; p < .05). The indirect effect of Behavioral Intention on Satisfaction with Decision was .213 and the total effect of Intention on Satisfaction was .213, indicating that Behavioral Intention had no direct effect on satisfaction. The variance in Satisfaction with Decision due to Behavioral Intention was due to the indirect effect of Intention via Behavior. 103 12121121. diz i l f r R i l Intent. Behav Sat Self- Outcm Subj. Barr. w/Dec EffT2 Exp Norm Intent. 0.000 Behav. —0.001 —0.001 Sat. w/ Dec —0.558 —0.280 —0.254 Self-Eff. 1.328 —0.345 -0.190 0.000 Outc. Exp 0.469 0.316 1.459 0.000 0.000 Subj Nrm 0.597 —0.104 1.076 0.000 0.000 0.000 Barriers -0.312 -0.192 -0.473 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 IobJelZ Intent Behav. Sat Self-Eff Outcm. Subj. Barr. w/ Dec. T2 Expect. Norm Intent. 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 -0.000 0.000 0.000 Behav. 4.392“ 0.000 0.000 0.000 -2.636 0.000 -3.934* Sat 0.000 4.937" 0.000 4.394" 0.000 0.000 0.000, w/ Dec *p < .05 104 Hypbtheeie 4 Hypothesis 4 stated that Self-efficacy expectations and Outcome Expectations would be correlated. A Pearson Correlation coefficient indicated that Self- Efficacy at Time 2 was correlated significantly (p < .05) with Outcome Expectations at Time 2 (r = .31) supporting the hypothesis. AddifioualAnalxoee It was observed that behavior was sigrnificantly related to self-efficacy at Time 3 but not at Times 1 or 2. The correlations between behavior and self- efficacy were significantly different for Time 1 and Time 3 (t = -2.0; df = 62; p < .05); Time 2 and Time 3 (t = -2.5; df = 64; p < .05, 2-tail) but nOt for Time 1 and Time 2 (t = .48; 62 df; ns). See Appendix H for sample calculations. I An analysis of variance was conducted to determine whether the correlation was due to selection bias. Were those who chose to visit their health care providers higher in self-efficacy at Time 2 than those who chose not to? There was no significant difference (F=.6506) in self-efficacy at Time 2 between women who visited their health care providers (n=66; m =8.0; s.d.=1.45) and women who did not visit their health care providers (n=182; m=8.2; s.d.=1.45) during the follow-up period. This finding confirmed that the observed relationship between self-efficacy at time 3 and behavior was not due to selection bias. The correlation between participation behavior and self-efficacy was significantly geater at Time 3 than at Time 2 and was not due to selection bias. This suggested that behavior affected self-efficacy. To determine the extent to which post-behavior self-efficacy was influenced by experience (behavior), a multiple regession was performed which controlled for self-efficacy at Times 1 and 2. Table 23 shows the regession coefficients and significance values for the regession equation. The multiple regession analysis showed that participation behavior explained 9% of the variance in self-efficacy at Time 3 when self- 105 efficacy at Times 1 and 2 were controlled. This supported the speculation that active participation behavior increased self-efficacy when pre-behavior levels of self-efficacy were controlled for. Figure 17 gaphically depicts the relationship between self-efficacy and behavior. Participation behavior is shown on the vertical y-axis (height) with self- efficacy at Time 3 plotted on the horizontal x-axis (width) and self-efficacy at Time 2 plotted on the z—axis represented by depth. The figure is rotated in space so that the moderate correlation between behavior and Time 3 self-efficacy is apparent. Note the elliptical pattern of the points plotted between the x and y axes. The relationship between behavior and self-efficacy at Time 2 can be seen by viewing the plot from front to back (depth). As can be seen, there is no pattern to the depth at which each point is plotted and its height on the vertical axis. At Time 2, most self-efficacy scores cluster near the high end of the scale, regardless of behavior. 8. v a... 3:36:08 and 8. .23 mm. . an. m. o3 H. a: B. 8323 m N 95. new 8. 83H 8. 6m. .88 Hm. 8H3»... G: on. 885928 N H 68:. .3me mm. .3: _E. am. new _3. Tm: mm T38. 883928 H cwmi «W... m m _HR no.3 n Eula 8.3 m ozaea> 68m 107 .14... >nm98 For13IszithN=156;8192 For15lewithN=156;7891 EQLSmallfiffectfize: r2 = .02; (See p. 413 Cohen, 1988) = n = .02 N=248;a2=.05;u=13;v=248-13-1=234 2. = .02 (13 + 234 + 1) = 4.96; 19 < [3 > 29 (See p. 421 Cohen, 1988) APPENDIX C p— O 148 APPENDIX C INTERVENTION OUTLINES Outline of Intervention A Session I Introduction and informed consent forms (5 minutes) a. Importance of study and data i. "Participants are an important part of this study and we need your help“ b. Discuss incentives to continue participation Data Collection (30 minutes) a Problem Significance Assessment b Sociodemographic c. Perceptions (Menopause Problem Scale and Control Scale) (1. Symptoms Instrument e. Symptom Management/Self-Care Instrument f. Knowledge Instrument g Satisfaction with Health Care Provider Instrument (Part I) h Satisfaction with Decision Instrument i. Health Care Self-efficacy Instrument Distribute Brochures Session 111 Personal Plan Form and Activity Record Calendar (15 Minutes) (Clinician) Data Collection (25 Minutes) a. Knowledge b Satisfaction with Decision c Health Care Self-Efficacy d. Barriers to Decision Scale e. Outcome Expectations Scale f. Subjective Norms Scale g. Behavioral Intentions Scale Emphasize the importance of data collection and returning in six months a. Discuss incentives to return calendars and return in six months and one year 149 Outline of Intervention B Session I Bold denotes topics in knowledge instrument 1. IntroduCtion and informed consent forms (5 minutes) 2. Data Collection (30 minutes) '5‘“? 2'7.“ 9‘9 57!” 1. Problem Significance Assessment Sociodemographic Perceptions (Menopause Problem Scale and Control Scale) Symptoms Instrument Symptom Management/Self-Care Instrument Knowledge Instrument Satisfaction with Health Care Provider Instrument (Part I) Satisfaction with Decision Instrument Health Care Self-efficacy Instrument 3. Physical changes of menopause (20 Minutes) (Clinician) [Participants may follow along in the brochure] What 13 menopause? i. Definition of menopause ii. Menopause is a normal transition iii. Changes at midlife iv. Surgical menopause When does menopause occur? 1. Average age of menopause is 50 What happens to my body at menopause? i. Childbearing years (1) Ovary contains follicles which decrease in number over life-span (2) Two glands 1n the body to know about (a) Pituitary (b) Ovary (i) Responds to the signals from the pituitary (ii) Produces two primary hormones l) estrogen 2) progesterone (3) Two hormones that drive the ovary to do its job (a) FSH (b) LH (4) Feedback cycle of hormonal changes during menstrual cycle 150 (5) Hormonal changes at menopause (6) Fwdback cycle of childbearing years interrupted (7) Levels of pituitary hormones rise (8) Decrease in production of estrogen and progesterone ii. Menstrual periods change (1) Pattern of change varies between women iii. Ovulation can occur and pregnancy can result even without a menstrual period (1) Should use birth control for one full year Will I experience symptoms at menopause? (Clinician) a. b. gar-spam Sheehy quote Hot flashes one of most common symptoms i. Related to estrogen deficiency 80 % of women experience symptoms 20% of women seek help for symptoms Description of experience of hot flashes Estrogen reduces hot flashes in 95 -96% of cases Other changes due to estrogen decrease i. breast ii. vaginal lining (1) lining becomes less thick (2) dryer, thinner, shorter (3) Can result in burning or stinging with urination (a) difficulty or pain with intercourse (4) Can result in increased chance of vaginal infections Other things which occur around the time of the menopause i. anxiety ii. weight gain 1ii. bloating iv. Not shown to be directly due to estrogen deficiency Increased Risks at Menopause (Clinician) a. b. Two diseases that increase: Osteoporosis and heart disease Osteoporosis i. Definition ‘ ii. Around 50,000 women die per year as a result of osteoporotic fractures iii. Fracture may result in nursing home placement iv. Osteoporosis a calcium disease and protein disease v. Osteoporosis accelerates rate of development at menopause vi. Women most at risk for fractures: (1) Thin, white women who smoke and do not exercise regularly 151 c. Heart Disease i. Major cause of death for men and women ii. 390,000 women per year die of heart disease - iii. Heart disease in women tends to increase in rate of development around the time of the menopause 6. Self-Care Strategies (10 Minutes) (Clinician) a. For hot flashes: i. Eat a good, healthy diet ' (1) - To help hot flashes decrease stimulatory types of foods such as spices ii. Wear layered clothing (1) Can take of layers if get hot b. Skin (like the bones) gets thinner and dryer around the menopause i. . Natural skin lubricants like lanolin or other non-perfumed lubricants may help c. Vaginal dryness i. Use water-soluble lubricant d. For many people these self-eare strategies are enough to take care of the symptoms and signs that occur at the time of the menopause but don’t do anything do anything about the long-term, killing complications of osteoporosis and coronary heart disease. 152 Intervention Outline B Session 11 Pros and Cons of Hormone Replacement Therapy (30 Minutes) (Clinician) Five areas to think about related to HRT (SCOBES) S--Symptoms (menopausal symptoms) C--Coronary heart disease O—- -Osteoporosis B--Breast cancer E--Endometrial cancer S--Side effects of HRT Definition of base rates Frequency of occurrence Important because it addresses "double what?" "What are these relative to each other?" Two main regimens a. i. ii. iii. iv. v. vi. i. ii. 1. ii. Estrogen- Only (1) Typical regimen (2) Whether it would be prescribed for you would depend on your personal history and medical examination (3) Side effects include: (a) headache (b) bloating (0) weight gain (d) breast tenderness (4) No resumption of cyclic bleeding with estrogen-only (5) No chance of a woman getting pregnant Estrogen combined with Progesterone (1) Typical regimens (2) Side effects (a) More women experience side effects with combination therapy than with estrogen-only (b) Side effects similar to those experienced with estrogen: (i) headache (ii) bloating (iii) weight gain (iv) breast tenderness d. 153 Impact of Hormone Replacement Therapy on factors Symptoms i. (1) (2) Recap of Base Rate Risk (a) Hot flashes 80% of women experience them but only 20% seek help for them (b) Vaginal atrophy-becomes dryer and more easily injured and may cause difficulty with intercourse Effect of HRT (a) Both ERT and PERT relieve hot flashes by about 90-95% (h) Both regimens relieve vaginal dryness (c) Clinical data has not found that ERT or PERT affect other symptoms such as mood swings Coronary Heart Disease (1) (2) Recap of Base Rate Risk (a) Most frequent killer of both men and women. 394,000 women die each year (b) Heart disease occurs somewhat later in women than in men, however. Effect of HRT (a) Estrogen-only cuts risk in half (b) Addition of progestogen has been questioned. (c) There are less data about the effect of progestogen. Most research has been done on .625 of estrogen Osteoporotic Fractures (1) (2) Recap of Base Rate Risk (a) 50,000 women die each year from complications of f ractures. (b) Fractures occur predominantly among women and increase greatly with age. By age 80, 1 out of every 3 women will have had a hip fracture. Of those hip fractures, 20% will result in death Effect of HRT (a) Risk '5 cut in half with ERT and PERT iv. 154 Breast Cancer (1) (2) (3) Base Rate risk (a) 41,000 women die each year from breast cancer (b) Every woman over age 50 ought to have a mammogram every year (c) Breast cancer is usually treated with surgery, and/or chemotherapy Effect of HRT (a) Do not have the bottom line on this (b) Some studies indicate an increased risk, some decreased risk, some no change in risk (i) Increase is usually 1.3 to 1.8 increased risk Important to know the following when you read reports of studies: (a) What kind of estrogen (b) Dose (c) Duration of use Endometrial Cancer (1) Base Rate Risk (a) Cancer of the uterus (b) 3,000 women die each year from endometrial cancer (c) Is related to hormone replacement therapy (d) Mortality related to endometrial cancer is smaller (e) Symptoms of endometrial cancer is bleeding (0 A woman who experiences bleeding after menopause should see her health care professional (g) Lower base rate (2) Effect of HRT (a) ERT increases risk 6-fold (b) PERT negates the increased risk. No increased risk ‘ (c) Diagnosed earlier (d) Occurrence increases but mortality has not Side effects (1) Experienced by more women more severely with PERT (2) Sequential PERT usually causes resumption of cyclic bleeding (3) Neither will allow you to become pregnant again 10. 155 Question and Answer Period (10 Minutes) (Clinician) Clusters Narrative (10 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Factors included in the study (1) Hot flashes (2) Osteoporosis (3) Endometrial Cancer (4) ERT vs PERT b. Women look at the pieces of information differently c. Three major groups of women 1. Group I: 120 Women highly influenced by hot flashes ii. Group II: Were interested in hot flashes and osteoporosis about equally and were concerned about endometrial cancer. iii. Group HI: (40) Hot flashes extremely important to them. Would not want to resume monthly blwding. d. The reason for presenting this information is that you may see yourself in one of these groups (give or take a little). It gives you an idea of how some other women have weighed these factors Risk Discussion (20 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Factors which can affect base rate risk 1. Fracture (1) Weight-bearing exercise (at least 3 times per week for 20 minutes or more) (2) Diabetes Mellitus (3) Age (4) White! N on-white (5) Smoking (6) Hypertension (7) Calcium intake (8) Over/Underweight (9) Oophorectomy ii. Heart Disease (1) Family History (2) Age (over 50) (3) Smoking , (4) Exercise (At least 3 times per week for 20 minutes or more) (5) Diabetes Mellitus (6) Hypertension iii. 156 Endometrial Cancer Risk (1) Overweight (15 pounds or more) (2) Never pregnant (3) Previous difficulty getting pregnant (4) Irregular periods (5) Excessive menstrual blwding 11. Assess personal values (10 Minutes) (Social Scientist) Factors to consider a. 1. ii. iii. iv. v. vi. Menopausal symptoms Fractures due to osteoporosis Heart disease Endometrial cancer Cyclic bleeding Possible side effects Other factors 157 Intervention Program B Session III 1. Personal Plan Form and Activity Record Calendar (15 Minutes) (Clinician) Communicating with Health Care Professionals (10 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Two main aspects of effective communication: i. Effective listening allows: (1) Carry out health care provider’s recommendations (2) Help you understand information (a) Example Effective information-giving and question-asking allows: (1) Give info health care provider needs to give best care (2) Get back answers that you understand b. Preparing for the visit i. ii. iii. iv. Allows effective use of appointment time Allows you to assemble the information your health care provider will need Indicate if extra time is needed for a visit when the appointment is made (1) Example See brochure for list of activities to prepare for visit c. During the visit i. ii. 1V. vi. vii. viii. At beginning say have questions Describe your symptoms specifically (1) Example (2) Describe: (a) duration of the symptom (b) ’ when it occurs Say what you think may be the cause of your problem Listen carefully to questions Answer the questions completely and directly Ask the questions you have prepared ahead of time (1) Example Write down the answers to questions and information you will need later (1) Example If your health care provider makes a recommendations and you know right off that this 1s something that you won t be able to do. (1) Examples 158 d. After the visit 1. Review the visit after you leave the office (1) When you get home, ask yourself if your questions were answered (a) If yes, carry out the recommendations (b) If your goals weren’t met you can: (i) Call your health care provider and ask for further information or clarification (ii) If you decide that you need to see a specialist, you can call up and ask for a referral (iii) Ask your friends for some suggestions of referrals to other health care providers. Data Collection (25 Minutes) Knowledge Satisfaction with Decision Health Care Self-Efficacy Barriers to Decision Scale Outcome Expectations Scale Subjective Norms Scale Behavioral Intentions Scale rhea-9.61» 159 Outline of Intervention C Session I Bold denotes topics in knowledge instrument y... Introduction and informed consent forms (5 minutes) Data Collection (30 minutes) 53‘2” rm 99 9‘? i. Problem Significance Assessment Sociodemographic Perceptions (Menopause Problem Scale and Control Scale) Symptoms Instrument Symptom Management/Self-Care Instrument Knowledge Instrument Satisfaction with Health Care Provider Instrument (Part I) Satisfaction with Decision Instrument Health Care Self-efficacy Instrument Physical changes of menopause (20 Minutes) (Clinician) [Participants may follow along in the brochure] a. What is menopause? 1. Definition of menopause ii. Menopause is a normal transition iii. Changes at midlife iv. Surgical menopause When does menopause occur? 1. Average age of menopause is 50 , What happens to my body at menopause? i. Childbearing years (1) Ovary contains follicles which decrease in number over life-span ~' (2) Two glands in the body to know about (a) Pituitary (b) Ovary (i) Responds to the signals from the pituitary (ii) Produces two primary hormones 1) estrogen 2) progesterone (3) Two hormones that drive the ovary to do its job (a) FSH 01) LB (4) Fwdback cycle of hormonal changes during menstrual cycle 160 (5) Hormonal changes at menopause (6) Feedback cycle of childbearing years interrupted (7) Levels of pituitary hormones rise (8) Decrease in production of estrogen and progesterone ii. Menstrual periods change (1) Pattern of change varies between women iii. Ovulation can occur and pregnancy can result even without a menstrual period (1) Should use birth control for one full year Will I experience symptoms at menopause? (Clinician) a. b. Orr-map Sheehy quote Hot flashes one of most common symptoms i. Related to estrogen deficiency 80% of women experience symptoms 20% of women seek help for symptoms Description of experience of hot flashes Estrogen reduces hot flashes in 95-96% of cases Other changes due to estrogen decrease 1. breast ii. vaginal lining (1) lining becomes less thick (2) dryer, thinner, shorter (3) Can result in burning or stinging with urination (a) difficulty or pain with intercourse (4) Can result in increased chance of vaginal infections Other things which occur around the time of the menopause i. anxiety ii. weight gain iii. bloating iv. Not shown to be directly due to estrogen deficiency Increased Risks at Menopause (Clinician) a. b. Two diseases that increase: Osteoporosis and heart disease Osteoporosis i. Definition ii. Around 50,000 women die per year as a result of osteoporotic fractures iii. Fracture may result in nursing home placement iv. Osteoporosis a calcium disease and protein disease v. Osteoporosis accelerates rate of development at menopause vi. Women most at risk for fractures: (1) Thin, white women who smoke and do not exercise regularly 6. 7. 161 Heart Disease i. Maior cause of death for men and women ii. 390,000 women per year die of heart disease iii. Heart disease in women tends to increase in rate of development around the time of the menopause Self-Care Strategies (10 Minutes) (Clinician) a. For hot flashes: i. Eat a good, healthy diet (1) To help hot flashes decrease stimulatory types of foods such as spices ii. Wear layered clothing ( 1) Can take of layers if get hot Skin (like the bones) gets thinner and dryer around the menopause i. Natural skin lubricants like lanolin or other non-perfumed lubricants may help Vaginal dryness i. Use water-soluble lubricant For many people these self-care strategies are enough to take care of the symptoms and signs that occur at the time of the menopause but don’t do anything do anything about the long-term, killing complications of osteoporosis and coronary heart disease. Pros and Cons of Hormone Replacement Therapy (30 Minutes) (Clinician) a. Five areas to think about related to HRT (SCOBES) i. S--Symptoms (menopausal symptoms) ii. C--Coronary heart disease iii. O--Osteoporosis iv. B--Breast cancer v. E--Endometrial cancer vi. S--Side effects of HRT Definition of base rates i. Frequency of occurrence ii. Important because it addresses ”double what?" ”What are these relative to each other?" 162 c. Two main regimens i. Estrogen-Only (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) Typical regimen Whether it would be prescribed for you would depend on your personal history and medical examination Side effects include: (a) headache (b) bloating ' (c) weight gain (d) breast tenderness No resumption , of cyclic blwding with estrogen-only No chance of a woman getting pregnant ii. Estrogen combined with Progesterone (1) (2) Typical regimens Side effects (a) More women experience side effects with combination therapy than with estrogen-only (b) Side effects similar to those experienced with estrogen: (i) headache (ii) bloating (iii) weight gain (iv) breast tenderness (1. Impact of Hormone Replacement Therapy on factors i. Symptoms (1) (2) Recap of Base Rate Risk (a) Hot flashes 80% of women experience them but only 20% seek help for them (b) Vaginal atrophy—becomes dryer and more easily iniured and may cause difficulty with intercourse Effect of HRT (a) Both ERT and PERT relieve hot flashes by about 90-95% (h) Both regimens relieve vaginal dryness (c) Clinical data has not found that ERT or PERT affect other symptoms such as mood swings iii. iv. 163 Coronary Heart Disease (1) Recap of Base Rate Risk (a) Most frequent killer of both men and women. 394,000 women die each year (b) Heart disease occurs somewhat later in women than in men, however. (2) Effect of HRT (a) Estrogen-only cuts risk in half (b) Addition of progestogen has been questioned. (c) There are less data about the effect of progestogen. Most research has been done on .625 of estrogen Osteoporotic Fractures (1) Recap of Base Rate Risk (a) 50,000 women die each year from complications of fractures. (b) Fractures occur predominantly among women and increase greatly with age. By age 80, 1 out of every 3 women will have had a hip fracture. Of those hip fractures, 20% will result in death (2) Effect of HRT (a) Risk is cut in half with ERT and PERT Breast Cancer (1) Base Rate risk (a) 41,000 women die each year from breast cancer (b) Every woman over age 50 ought to have a mammogram every year (c) Breast cancer is usually treated with surgery, and/or chemotherapy (2) Effect of HRT (a) Do not have the bottom line on this (b) Some studies indicate an increased risk, some decreased risk, some no change in risk (i) Increase is usually 1.3 to 1.8 increased risk (3) Important to know the following when you read reports of studies: (a) What kind of estrogen (b) Dose (c) Duration of use Ve vi. 164 Endometrial Cancer (1) Base Rate Risk (a) Cancer of the uterus (b) 3,000 women die each year from endometrial cancer (c) Is related to hormone replacement therapy ((1) Mortality related to endometrial cancer is smaller (e) Symptoms of endometrial cancer is bleeding (1') A woman who experiences bleeding after menopause should see her health care professional (g) Lower base rate (2) Effect of HRT (a) ERT increases risk (Hold (1)") PERT negates the increased risk. No increased risk (c) Diagnosed earlier (d) Occurrence increases but mortality has not Side effects (1) Experienced by more women more severely with PERT (2) Sequential PERT usually causes resumption of cyclic bleeding ' (3) Neither will allow you to become pregnant again p—s 0 PM“? 165 Intervention Outline C Session II Question and Answer Period (10 Minutes) (Clinician) Clusters Narrative (10 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Aspects to combine into decision i. Your risk ii. What’s important to you b. Factors included in the study (1) Hot flashes (2) Osteoporosis (3) Endometrial Cancer (4) ERT vs PERT c.‘ Women look at the pieces of information differently (1. Three major groups of women i. Group 1:120 Women highly influenced by hot flashes ii. Group 11: Were interested in hot flashes and osteoporosis about equally and were concerned about endometrial cancer. iii. Group III: (40) Hot flashes extremely important to them. Would not want to resume monthly bleeding. e. The reason for presenting this information is that you may see yourself in one of these groups (give or take a little). It gives you an idea of how some other women have weighed these factors Personal Risk Assessment (20 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Complete forms i. Bar length represents chance of dying W ii. Bar length shows the chance of this being your cause of death (not necessarily soon) iii. Questions on risk assessment represent factors which can modify base rate iv. The risk assessment is simply a guide; estimates b. Transfer to the relevance chart Problem Significance Assessment (10 Minutes) (Social Scientist) Relevance Chart (15 Minutes) (Social Scientist) Scenarios (30 Minutes) (Clinician) a. Case 1 b. Case 2 Decision (Clinician) 166 Session III 1. Personal Plan Form and Activity Record Calendar (15 Minutes) (Clinician) Communicating with Health Care Professionals (10 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Two main aspects of effective communication: 1. ii. Effective listening allows: (1) Carry out health care provider’s recommendations (2) Help you understand information (a) Example Effective information-giving and question-asking allows: (1) Give info health care provider needs to give best care (2) Get back answers that you understand b. Preparing for the visit 1. ii. iii. iv. Allows effective use of appointment time Allows you to assemble the information your health care provider will need Indicate if extra time is nwded for a visit when the appointment is made (1) Example See brochure for list of activities to prepare for visit c. During the visit 1. ii. iii. iv. vi. vii. viii. At beginning say have questions Describe your symptoms specifically (1) Example (2) Describe: (a) duration of the symptom (b) when it occurs Say what you think may be the cause of your problem Listen carefully to questions Answer the questions completely and directly Ask the questions you have prepared ahead of time (1) Example Write down the answers to questions and information you will need later (1) Example If your health care provider makes a recommendations and you know right off that this is something that you won’t be able to do. (1) Examples 167 d. After the visit i. Review the visit after you leave the office (1) When you get home, ask yourself if your questions were answered (a) If yes, carry out the recommendations (b) If your goals weren’t met you can: (i) Call your health care provider and ask for further information or clarification (ii) If you decide that you need to see a specialist, you can call up and ask for a referral (iii) Ask your friends for some suggestions of referrals to other health care providers. Communicating with Health Care Provider Role Play ( 15 Minutes) (Social Scientist) a. Instructions: i. For those of you who are playing health care providers, remember you have a schedule to keep and patients in the waiting room but you are seeing one of your regular patients. ii. For those of you who are playing patients, you have called ahead - and informed the receptionist you want to make an appointment to discuss menopause b. Afterwards: i. For those who were playing health care providers, how did it go? How did it work to have this patient in here asking you questions? ii. For those who were patients, how did it go? Data Collection (25 Minutes) Knowledge Satisfaction with Decision Health Care Self—Efficacy Barriers to Decision Scale Outcome Expectations Scale Subjective Norms Scale Behavioral Intentions Scale erase-.0571» APPENDD( D 168 APPENDIX D MEASURES ID 1' DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY Mark your answer for each question on the answer sheet provided. These sheets will be scored by machine so it is very important that you completely darken in your answer choice in the proper location on the answer sheet with the pencil provided. Do not leave any stray pencil marks on the sheet outside of your answer space and makesure . ‘1]... ° 1 1. "11 .-H-. .".\ -. e . U'I°I W. For example, consider this question: The first month of the year is: l = March 2 = July 3 = January 4 = October To choose January, you should darken in 3 on your answer sheet as is done below. 2 assaeaaaba At the top of each page is a reminder to check to be sure you are filling in the correct row on your answer sheet for the questions. This publication is part of 'Women's Judgments of Estrogen Replseemelu Therapy." which is supported by Grant 0R0! NR0124S-04A2 from the Nations] Carter for Nursing Resesrweh. Nelsons! last'sues of Health. its com are solely the responsibility oflher ashore and bumflywtheoffical vaewsoflheNstiealeeuerfor Nuniagllesareh. 169 DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY BARRIERS TO PARTICIPATION IN HEALTH CARE DECISIONS Some people want to help make decisions about their health are but an not. Other factors affect how much input they an have in decisions. These questions are about how much you feel that these factors WWW. How much do these factors limit your participation in medial decisions? Use the scale below: l 2 3 4 5 Not at All Somewhat A Great Deal 6 = NOT APPLICABLE 24. How much does your doctor (or regular health are practitioner) limit your participation in medial decisions? 25. How much do your payment options (such as health insurance) limit your participation in medial decisions? 26. How much does your health state or personal risk factors limit your participation in medial decisions? . 27. How much doeslack ofaccess to accurate information limit your participation in medial decisions? 28. How much does conflicting medial advice limit your participation in medial decisions? 29. How much do other aspects of the health are system limit your participation in medial decisions? Plase specify: 30. How much do other factors limit your participation in medial decisions? Please specify: ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 170 DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY OUTCOME EXPECTATIONS SCALE These statements are about patient involvement in health are decisions in general. W mfonmunnmionmthcunxnlxcmcnuimbuhmem Then statements are about the outcome of patient participation, that is, W that patient participation will result in the outcomes described? Please answer questions 31-37 using the following response sale: 1 2 3 4 5 Extremely Unlikely Neither Likely Likely Extremely Unlikely Nor Unlikely Likely Patient participation in medial decisions and choices results in: 31. Medial procedures that are unnecessary for the patient 32. Medial procedures that are harmful for the patient 33. The wrong mediations for the paient 34. The right medial tests and examinations for the patient 35. The right treatment for the patient 36. The right medial decisions for the patient 37. The best medial are Outcome Values Sale Pleaseusethefollowingsaletoanswerquestions38-44. Foreachoutcome,indiatehowgoodorhad itwouldbeform: 1 2 3 4 5 Extremely Bad Neither Bad Good Extremely Bad Nor Good Good 38. Medial procedures that are unnecessary for me 39. Medial procedures that are harmful for me 40. The wrong mediations for me ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 171 Check your answer sheet. You should now be filling in row 41 Please use the following sale to answer questions 4144. For each outcome, indieate how good or bad it would be for you: 1 2 3 4 5 Extremely Bad Neither Bad Good Extremely Bad Nor Good . Good 41. 'I‘herightmedialtestsandexaminationsforme 42. The right treatment for me 43. The right medial decisions for me 44. The best medical are ° 1992 Michigan State University CollegeofNursing 172 DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY SUBJECI'IVE NORMS QUESTIONNAIRE Some people think that the decision to take hormone replacement therapy should be made by the clinician while other people think that the decision should be made by the patient. Indiate how you believe each of the following people think the decision should be made. Use the scale below to answer questions 45-49. (Clinician means your doctor or regular health are practitioner). 1 2 3 4 5 Clinician Clinician After Clinician Patient After Patient Considering and Patient Considering 5 = NOT APPUCABLE Patient's Opinion Together Equally Clinician’s Opinion 45. Your doctor (or regular health are practitioner) thinks the decision should be made by: 46. Your spouse, partner or signifiant other thinks the decision should be made by: 47. Your parents, relatives and children think the decision should be made by: 48. Your friends, peers and classmates think the decision should be made by: 49. Other people you think influence your decision (Please specify: ) think the decision should be made by: MOTIVATION TO COMPLY Please use the following scale to answer questions 50-54 below. L J 3 4 5 5 = NOT APPLICABLE Not at All Very Much 50. Howmuchdoyouwanttodowhatyourdoctor(orregularhealtharepractitioner)wantsyouto. 51. How much do you want to do what your spouse, partner or signifant other wants you to do? 52. How much do you want to do what your parents, relatives and children want you to do? 53. How much do you want to do what your friends, peers or classmates want you to do? 54. How much do you want to do what the other persons you listed above want you to do? (Please specify: ) ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 173 DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY BEHAVIORAL INTENTIONS QUESTIONNAIRE To what extent do you man or plan to do the following activities related to your next visit to your doctor (or regular health are practitioner)? Please use the following sale to answer questions 55-62. 2 3 4 5 Not At All Completely 55. Carefully consider and understand my mum related to the tradeoffs of hormone replacement therapy or other are. 56. Gather the information I need to make an informed decision about hormone replacement therapy or other are. 57. Carefully consider my W513: related to hormone replacement therapy or other are. 58. Ask for an extended appointment to discuss my concerns about my health are. 59. Prepareformyvisitbythinkingaboutthequestionslwanttoask,Wandbring the list with me to the visit. 60. At the beginning of the visit, tell my doctor/practitioner that 1 have questions I would like to discuss. 61. Duringmyvisit,askthequestionslhadprepared,repeattheanswerstobesurelunderstand 62. Aftermy visit,reviewthevisit,followthetratmentsorrecommendationsagreeduponandall my doctor/practitioner if I have additional questions or unexpected side effects. 63. Regarding the decision whether or not to take hormone replacement therapy, how do you Worplanthatthedecisionwillbemade?chmmne. (Clinician meansyourdoctoror regular health are practitioner). 1- Iintendtohavemyelinicianmakethedecision,usingallthat'slmownabouthormone replacement therapy. 2 - I intend to have my clinician make the decision but strongly consider my opinion. 3 a lintend that myclinicianandlwillmakethedecision together, onanequal basis. 4 - I intend to make the decision myself but stongly consider the clinicians’opinion. 5 - I intend to make the decision myself, using all I know or learn about hormone replacement therapy ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 174 DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY HEALTH CARE SELF-EFFICACY MEASURE Beloware8sinrationsyoumightexperienceasapatient. Onasaleof1t010,circlethe numbewhichbefldamibahowanfidatyouwouflfedmyomabflitywhandlemesimadon mm. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident HOW CONFIDENT ARE YOU- 1. —1hatwhayouneedmedialareyouanpmvideimportantinputaboutwhatwinbe doneto you? l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident 2. --In your ability to understand any medial procedures which might be done to you? l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident 3. -That when you need medial are, your input would help you get the best are? l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident 4. -That once a health professional has begun doing a medial procedure with you, you can ask important questions about the procedure? l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident 175 -That you would know how to help your health are provider decide what medial procedures you should get? 1 . 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident «Thatyouanaskyourhalthareprovider important questions about your healthafter amedialexambeforeyouaretold? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident . Extremely Confident -‘1'hatyouwouldbeabletounderstand information about whatamedial procedure woulddotoyou? l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident -That, given many choices about what would be good for your health, you could make the best choice? l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not at all Confident Extremely Confident 176 DECISION MAKING IN MENOPAUSE STUDY SATISFACTION WITH DECISION MAKING PROCESS INSTRUMENT You have been considering whether to consult your health care provider about hormone replacement therapy. Answer the following questions about your decision. Please indicate to what extent each statement is true for you AT THIS TIME by circling your answer. 1. I am satisfied that I am adequately informed about the issues important to my decision. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree ' Agree Strongly Disagree . Nor Disagree Agree 2. The decision I made was the best decision possible for me personally. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree ' Nor Disagree Agree 3. I am satisfied that my decision was consistent with my personal values. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 4. I expect to successfully carry out (or continue to carry out) the decision I made. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree 5. I am satisfied that this was my decision to make. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 10. 177 I am satisfied with my decision. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree . Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree UNDERSTANDING MY DECISION This decision is hard for meto make. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree It’s clear what choice is best for me. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree I’m unsure what to do in this decision. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree - I know all the possible choices open to me in protecting my health after menopause . Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree ' Agree ° 1992 Michigan State University L College of Nursing ll. 12. 13. 14. 178 I understand the risks and benefits of taking hormone replacement therapy. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree RESTRICTIONS My health state restricted my decision related to hormone replacement therapy. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree My health state restricted my decision related to calcium intake. Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree My health state restricted my decision related to exercise. Strongly Disagree , Neither Agree . Agree Strongly Disagree Nor Disagree Agree ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 179 15. On a scale of 1 to 10 with 1 = No confidence at all and 10 = complete confidence, circle the number which indicates your confidence in the correctness of your decision. 1 signifies no confidence at all. 10 signifies complete confidence; 2 through 9 indicate some middle level of confidence. 12 3 4 5 6 7 8 910 No Confidence Complete at All Confidence ° 1992 Michigan State University College of Nursing 180 Behavior Self-Report . Please answer the following questions WW brochure. If you have not visited your health care provider since completing the program, please check here: and skip these questions. To what extent did you do the following activities related to your last visit to your doctor (or regular health care practitioner)? 1. I carefully considered and understood my nines related to- the tradeoffs of hormone replacement therapy or other care. 1 2 3 4 5 Not At All _ Completely 2. I gathered the information I needed to make an informed decision about hormone replacement therapy or other care. 1 2 ' 3 4 5 Not At All - Completely 3. I carefully considered my mum related to hormone replacement therapy or other care. 1 2 3 4 5 Not At All Completely 181 I asked for an extended appointment to discuss my concerns about my health care. 1 2 3 4 5 Not At All Completely I prepared for my visit by thinking about the questions I wanted to ask, W and bringing the list with me to the visit. 1 2 . 3 - 4 5 Not At All , Completely At the beginning of the visit, I told my doctor/practitioner that I had questions I would like to discuss. l 2 3 4 - . 5 Not At All Completely During my visit, I asked the questions I had prepared, repeated the answers to be sure I understood them and WW necessary- 1 2 3 4 5 Not At All Completely After the visit, I reviewed the visit, followed the treatments or recommendations agreed upon and called my practitioner if 'I had additional questions or experienced unexpected side effects. I‘J U h 1 5 Not At All Completely 182 Which of the following choices best describes how the decision about whether or not you would take hormone replacement therapy mug made? (Clinician means your doomr or other health care practitioner). (Circle One). - , 1 I- The clinician made the decision, using all that’s known about hormone replacement therapy. - The clinician made the decision but strongly considered my opinion. The clinician and I made the decision together, on an equal basis. I made the decision, but strongly considered the clinician’s opinion. I made the decision using all I know and learned about hormone replacement therapy. Not.Applicable. I: am not yet experiencing menopause. 2 MAN ll llll II a ll APPENDD( E 183 ' Appendix E Scale Means and Standard Deviations Scale It Mean SD. Self-Efficacy T1 244 7.6 1.67 Self-Efficacy T2 248 8.1 1.45 Self-Efficacy T3 184 8.1 1.31 Barriers 247 2.32 .77 Outcome Expectations 248 18.5 3.60 Subjective Norm 243 10.5 3.67 Behavioral Intention 248 4.4 .60 Behavior Self-Report 67 3.8 .83 ‘ Satisfaction with Decision T3 184 4.0 .63 187 ‘ APPENDIX F 184’ APPENDIX F Calculation of Correction for Unreliability 1' = 1' XYtrue xy obt an a” r = True correlation xy true r xy obt «xx = Reliability of measure x = Observed correlation ‘Eyy = Reliability of measure y From : Edwards, A- L. (1954). Statisficalmeflmdaionthehehaxioralsciences New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. 185 ' APPENDD( G 185 ‘ APPENDIX G COMPUTATION S FOR REGRESSION From Cohen 8: Cohen (1983); p. 329: Y=wa+ Bcc+ Bee+ chwc+ Bwewe+ A Where: w = A quantitative variable (Self-Efficacy at Time 1) c, e = A set of contrast-coded variables representing the variance in a nominal variable with 3 experimental groups (Contrast 1, Contrast 2) wc, we = A set of variables representing the variance due to the interaction between the contrast-coded experimental group and the quantitative variable (Interaction 1, Interaction 2) A = Constant Calculations: Equationl Y = .57(w) + .15(c) + 1.12(e) - .03(wc) — .13(we) + 3.79 where: Y = Self-Efficacy at Time 2 w = Self-Efficacy at Time 1 c = Contrast 1 [Group C (+1) against Groups A (—.5) and B (-.5) pooled] e = Contrast 2 [Group B (+1) against Group A (-1)] wc = Interaction 1 [Product of Self-Efficacy at Time 1 and Contrast Variable 1] we = Interaction 2 [Product of Self-Efficacy at Time 1 and Contrast Variable 2] A = Constant 186 ‘ Equation 1 Reordered: Y = .57(w) - .03(wc) — .13(we) + .15(c) + 1.12(e) + 3.79 = [.57 — .03(c) — .13(e)]w + .15(c) + 1.12(e) + 3.79 Substituting coding for Experimental Group: A YA = [.57 — .03(—.5) — .13(—1)]w + .15(—.5) + 1.12(—1) + 3.79 = .715(SET1) + 2.595 9, = [.57 — .03(—.5) — .13(+1)]w + .15(—.5) + 1.12(-1) + 3.79 = .455(SET1) + 4.835 YC = [.57 - .03(+1 )- .13(O)]w + .15(+1) + 1.12(0) + 3.79 = .540(SET1) + 3.940 APPENDD( H 187 APPENDIX H Significance Test for Correlated r’ s Correlationatnl . Self-Eff 1 Self-Eff 2 Self-Eff 3 Self-Eff 2 .66 (65) Self-Eff 3 .50 .52 (65) (67) Behavior .10 .05 .34 (65) (67) (67) Correlation: Behavior & Self-Efficacy Time 2 = rxy = .05; n=67 Correlation: Behavior & Self-Efficacy Time 3 = rvy = .34; n=67 Correlation: Self-Efficacy Time 2 8: Self-Efficacy Time 3 = rxv = .52; n=66 IRI = 1—r2xy—r2‘fy-r2xv+2rxyrvyrxv E: r :r t=(rxv-rw)‘l(n-1)(1+r) \/2C- 191111.; (1 1.3) I R l = 1 — (.05)2— (.34)2— (.52)2+ 2(.05)(.34)(.52) = .63 ?=.05+.34 t=(.—05 .-34)~J(67 1)(1+.5'2' ¢2 (667-31) .63+(.2) (1- .52)3 7.. From: Cohen,]., 8: Cohen, P. (1983). (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Pp. 56—57.