[Ii-:1“; (“.L‘0 h“ l:l‘;5; ":53 g E‘ ..o gum: rd“) 5 f *0 fl ‘ II- -. “\fl‘ ‘-- 5 -: 0 lgH’lu‘l‘ up“ 5'5": . “m “#15?" 555:». , . ”\‘W‘w.’ .J:-. A 1 v ‘3'. "27' 7 14‘. 5mm; "5' “i z.. 103‘“; .415“. I M} h 5 r. ‘5 [fivfil‘ ‘” ' 5 ’IIW {51 7.155515"? 5 5" ’fr' .641 1 ' - . ' ‘ 6 ‘:§\:: 1 ‘1 5 5 d5. :'-.":":,’;‘~5“-"’ i1 Ta ' KW 55? 3::‘1'Va‘s b’ .I5‘:~f'figq5y|¥ 5551 5'15 . 5555'... a m. (32) Editfiflgfl' I r I! 5.5555 :tT II} - I». 5555' ‘ ID I 575.3%! “‘1 v f. ‘- . . 54b! 1.1.3} D, .du .51» .‘ , I" ’.. I' 0 '33, v—{ T.- ”A A A".< c... .0‘ —' t. —._. 2.2.“ '- _. ‘3‘?" '. -—- {.3 g ’1’ ~ .2 ~ s. ' .‘ - t ‘M 0* -':-I .. .. 5-7341 ‘5' “is: z 41-" -, -v.- . -I H H. ~ 4 .- QC .424 —.- J. Ovo £52.. ,1, IM- 1:“5537 .‘f—n I —I ~q. I . A ‘. .9 . iv Is. I. . “my? "mt-r. '5' IX'. ' I ‘ ‘2‘; {E5 r_ . . I'm}. ”A; - - g' 5 :33. ._. _: 7:332:43? flgfffifi‘é‘wg 35"55'5‘1‘53" ‘ 5‘ 11‘": 335; 3;» 'x‘qqy ”45511.5?” ~-‘ 9?”; 5555,51!" 1-";::‘ £65“: 4' a: :15ng eg- I :I’ . FIQ‘ I 5. 7} :5- 315":7§‘o3»§~.5§" . 1525:; ‘ . ‘ " “ ' ”#5555555 .47“ ' . ézfif! ~ . ._.. a: 56” 5“ ‘ e . org} .' ' 5 H '- 43;.“ I: “m 7.x; . .3;— Ava-31‘ \ . {.th 5‘ a ‘ u C «.2 o; . u_ . 1.. . 14-5-2. I. W. I I‘—_“, {.2} “ Q. L: ' 3:505: ‘fl’h: ' «:33? 5.55:" ' ‘5 I'I'EEI": 5‘55 I: 1. I" l .. ' '. “ .. 55:53: réh 0‘ ‘53; 7?; ”I ._ “dill” xi! “I“; “f Ar 1|} 5:5 i‘ 25",“ 'Il."‘ I‘ ,i V. “9275.5: -‘I r 'I:- . L-"z‘ 5?;1.IU'5:'5'1:I-5I’"'I .!‘_' 4453531355}.I:#r'1~,‘flf‘f§‘;i " 1 5w I.r-::n..virq‘ 1'! FjI 519‘ “will . ‘ . . 0 5-7:.» 2‘ .[ {Y 'I' FW‘L 5‘ v ‘1"‘555'5- .{ ‘3’ ”155;“ 51“ :3: 15' D55“ AMDJIQM “hmbfil $5555.55 .I.‘ 34.51;! V551,; 9' ..3, ‘I v' \‘ ‘,N «1;. “‘3 H... - .5“: .0 :w": In... 3.07:.” IIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIII 1293 01063 202 28 UBRARY _ Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE RELATION BETWEEN "PAIHocmESIs'm PARENTS AND RORSCHACH INDICES OF EGO FUNCI'IONING IN PREADOIESCEM‘ DAUGHTERS presented by LINDA DIANE LEIGH has been accepted towards fulfillment - of the requirements for Ph. D. degree in Psychology k/L Major professor Bertram Karon Date 4‘ ’ /{/’d06 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 MSU RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LJBRARJES remove this checkout from .—,—. your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. L2. 1 I ‘- . A . ‘ ”‘L. is, Mb- 6 m: ‘3.le O 8 2.304 lotiumzwglooa THE RELAITONIBETWEEN "PflEHCGENESIS" IN PARENTS AND RORSCHACH INDICES OF EGOIFUNCTIONING IN PREADOEESCENT’DAUGHTERS BY Linda Diane Leigh AIDISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of m or Panosam Department of Psychology 1986 ABSTRACT 'IHE RELATION BEIWEDJ "PA'I'HGSENESIS" IN PARENIS AND RORSZHPCH INDICES CF EEO FUICTIQ‘JIM; IN WENT DAUGHTERS By Linda Diane Leigh The purpose of this investigation was to clarify the relationship between the ”pathogenesis" of parents and the ego functioning of their preadolescent daughters. Ego functioning was assessed in a nonclinical group of 20 lZ-year-old females, whose parents ranged from low to high in ”pathogenesis." The pathogenic parent is one who is psychologically destructive when in the dominant position in a relationship in which needs of the individuals conflict, and will satisfy his/her needs without regard for the needs of the dependent person. Pathogenesis was determined by the Pathogenesis Index (PI) (Meyer & Karon, 1967) used on the TAT stories given by each parent. The following Rorschach factors reflect aspects of the daughters' ego functioning: disordered thinking (12D, proportion of disorganized to organized coping resources @1135) . number of responses (8) , human movement (H). proportion of appropriate pure form (m3 , proportion of good developmental quality (119.) (Exner, 1974), anxious content (AL) and hostile content (EL) (mizer, 1949). An observational measure was provided by teachers' ratings of the daughters' behavior. Correlations were computed between the daughters' Rorschach scores and the mothers' PI, fathers' PI, and average parental PI. 'Peachers' ratings were correlated with all of the above factors. Parental pathogenesis correlated positively with indices of poor ego functioning in the daughters. Significant correlations, in the expected direction, were found between mothers' PI and 1>'_I‘_. EL, AL, £9138. B. and m; fathers' PI and 121, EL, AL, m, and m; and average parental PI and 121:, EL, AL, @155, B, E, and m. 'Ihe teachers' ratings correlated significantly in the expected direction with mothers‘ PI, fathers' PI, average PI, and fran the daughters' Rorschach protocols: 121, EL, AL. 3, and £13,. As predicted maternal pathogenecity correlated positively with daughters' ego disturbance. Paternal pathogenecity produced stronger correlations than predicted with several indices of daughters' ego functioning. Significant correlations in the expected direction increased when the index of pathogenesis was the average of both parents. mm 'Ihere are a nuxrber of persons who have been instrumental in the development and completion of my dissertation. My thanks go to Drs. Joseph Reyher, Albert Rabin, and Dozier Thornton for serving so capably on my cannittee, and to my friend and colleague, Lisa Cohen, for her assistance in scoring the Rorschach protocols and TAT stories. A special acknowledgment is extended to Dr. Bertram Karon, my conmittee chairman, for his supervision of my research. ii WWW LIST m mm. 0 O O O O O O 0 LIST (I FIGURES . . . . . . . . WW . . . . . . . . . RINIEWW'IHELITERA'IURE. . . . Parental Pathogenesis. . . Ego Functioning in the Child Projective Tests . . . . . Interpretation of the Rorschach Variables. StatementofPurpose........... S'IWIWI‘CFHYPUIEESES. . . . mwooooooooooooo SUbjmtS O O O I O O O I 0 Procedure. . . . . . . . Influence of Extraneous Variables. . mm 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Analysis of the Relationship between Interscorer Reliability and Internal Consistency 'IIeachers ' Ratingsand Other Variables . . . . . . . . Analysis of the Relationship between Parents Pathogenesis Scores and Daughters' Rorschach $01.83..oooooooooooooooooo 9.mmaryofResults............... DImSSIm O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I 0 Relationship between Parents' PI, Daughters' Rorschach Scores and Teachers' Ratings. Support for Mahler's. Masterson's, and Lidz's'lheories. . . . . . . . . . . Parental Pathogenesis. . . . . . . . . . . Signs of Disturbed Ego Functioning in Four Rorschach Protocols . . . . . . . . . WANDCCI‘UMSICNS............ mm A. O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 iii Page vi 16 18 35 37 39 39 44 44 49 51 52 58 59 60 63 64 69 74 7'7 mm B. APPENDIX C. APPENDIX D. APPEN'JIX E. APPENDH F. APPENDIX G. APPENDIX H. iv 79 82 83 84 86 88 89 LIST (F MES Table 1 Rorschach Indices of Ego Functioning and Hypothesized Relationships to Parental Pathogenesis. 2 Correlations between Rorschach R and 21:, my AL; Mlfllnlmooooooooooooooooo 3 Correlations between Parental PI Scores and Daughters' Intelligence Test Scores. . . . . . . . 4 Correlations between 'Ibachers' Ratings and Rorschach Variables, PI, Intelligence Scores, SES. . . . . . 5 Correlations between PI Scores and SES . . . . . . 6 Correlations between Daughters' Rorschach Scores and Parents' Pathogenesis Score. . . . . . . . . . 7 Test of Difference between Mothers' and Fathers' Pathogenesis................... 8 Descriptive Data on the Subject Population . . . . A1 The Thorndike Vocabulary Test. . . . . . . . . . . A2 Daughters' Rorschach Scores Correlated with Pathogenesis Scores (SIB controlled). . . . . Page 38 46 47 47 48 52 54 86 87 LIST CF FIGURES Figure Page 1 The relationship between daughters' human movement response and average parental pathogenesis score. . . . . . 55 vi INTRODUCTION The purpose of this investigation is to clarify the relationship between the "pathogenesis" of parents and the ego functioning of their preadolescent daughters. ”Pathogenesis" is defined as the destructive attitude of the dominant.member of a pair toward the dependent member. By destructive attitude we mean that the lack of consideration to the needs and wishes of the dependent individual interferes with that individual's normal ego development. In some cases interference may be so severe that schizophrenia results. This investigation is directed toward determining the level of ego functioning in twelve-year-old daughters that correspond to various levels of pathogenesis in their parents. The nature of the problem upon which the present study is focused is whether higher levels of parental pathogenesis are associated with poorer ego functioning and lower levels of parental pathogenesis are associated with more adequate ego functioning in the daughters. It is a.question ofIWhether there is a positive correlation between parental pathogenesis and indices of poor ego functioning in the daughters. Parental pathogenesis is revealed by the Pathogenesis Index (PI) (Meyer & Karon, 1967), a scale used for scoring Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) (Mnrray, 1943) stories. Rorschach (Rorschach, 1921) performance has been selected as the means to evaluate the daughters' level of ego functioning. Rorschach measures of reality testing (Fifi), mature cognition (DQ), adaptiveness and productivity (8), ability to 1 delay impulse discharge and utilize fantasy for the expression of inner drives (31) , the proportion of disorganized to organized coping resources Leg/EA), disordered thinking (In) (Ether, 1974), anxiety level (AL), and hostility level (m0 (Elizur, 1949) point to various aspects of the daughters' current ego functioning. An observational measure provides a validity check on the projective instruments and a further test of the relationship between pathogenesis of the parents and their daughters' ego functioning. Each daughter is assessed by her teachers regarding how closely her performance matches her estimated abilities. REVIEW (F THE LITERATURE W The pathogenic parent concept has developed from the psychodynamic view of schizophrenia as a reaction to disturbed family relations which result in the experience of overwhelming anxiety during childhood (Arieti, 1955) . Ebrperiences with the mother are the most important influence on the infant, and these experiences diminish in importance only gradually through infancy and childhood as the child is able to separate and individuate. In support of this concept, Framr—Reiclmann (1960) uncovered feelings of distrust and resentment of other people in her schizophrenic patients. She contended that schizophrenia was due to the severe early destructive experiences including rejection which the patient encountered in the important peOple during infancy and childhood. Often it was due to the mother's influences. The pathogenic mother interferes with normal object relations for the child on whom her pathology is focused (Alanen, 1960) . This interference is apparent in a study by Wynne and Singer (1963), in which families of adolescent and young adult schizophrenics were assessed with the TAT and Rorschach. It was found that parents of schizophrenics were vindictive toward their offspring when s/he tried to establish a relationship with someone other than them. This research also points to the connection between the schizophrenic offspring's style of thinking and that of the parents, especially the mother. The pathogenic mother is overtly benign; however there is 3 a destructive attitude that is difficult to clarify, and the schizophrenic's feelings toward the mother figure seem to be very ambivalent (Lu, 1961). Lidz (1965) contends that the early family environment is a critical force in personality development both normal and abnormal. Few factors can affect personality development as much as intrafamilial relationships. Here the basic attitudes toward later interpersonal relationships are established: the formation of the projective systems by which the individual perceives the world is begun. The child receives his basic training in the meaning of words and in ways of communicating, reasoning, and thinking within his/her family of origin. The individual's reaction in all subsequent group and interpersonal situations rest upon the foundations provided within the family. Yet the family has its own needs, which may take precedence over its educational functions. It may be deviant from the remainder of society, set conflicting demands upon the child, provide inconsistent emotional experiences, teach paralogical ways, and in many other areas provide faulty schooling. Although the family is not the only influence upon theIdeveloping ego, it provides the most important set of influences impinging on the child (Lidz et a1., 1965). The earliest mother-child relationships during the oral phase of development seem to have critical significance for the development of severe psychopathology. However, the cardinal symptomatology of schizophrenia seem to indicate that later developmental periods also have critical significance. The symbolic distortions, the interpenetration of reality by fantasy, the confusion of sexual identity, the concerns over incestuous impulses, the attribution of omniscience and omnipotence to parental figures are all significant 4 problems of the early oedipal or late preoedipal phases of development. Although disturbances in the oral phase may establish a potential for schizophrenia, studies by Lidz and his colleagues (1965) of the families that produce schizophrenic offspring show that family relationships had been abnormal from birth through adolescence (Lidz et a1., 1965). Lidz (1973) contends that any attempt to study the child‘s ego development as an autonomous process independent of the family matrix distorts as much as it simplifies for such abstractions can be made only by eliminating the essential factors in the process. The child does not grow up to attain a mature workable personality simply through the nurturance of inborn directives and potentialities, but requires positive direction and guidance in a suitable interpersonal environment and social system. S/he does not develop into a well-integrated, adaptable adult simply because fixations have not occurred due to some innate tendency, some traumatic event, or some flow in maternal nurturance during the preoedipal phases of development. The positive molding forces have been largely overlooked because they are built into the institutions and mores of all societies, and into the family which has carried out the task of directing the child's potential into an integrated structure. The family must foster and direct the child's development through its nurturant functions, the influence of the family organization, the ways it functions as a social system, and through its transmission of the instrumented techniques of culture, particularly its language with its systems of meanings and logic. As an example of some of the qualities found in a pathogenic parent, Lidz (1973) describes a.mother of a schizophrenic whose 5 difficulties in communicating appeared clearly on her projective tests. She gave extremely vague responses, blurred meanings constantly, and became even more elusive when questioned about her responses. The responses she gave when she first saw a Rorschach card changed or disappeared when she was asked about them on the "inquiry." She conveyed that meanings were scarcely worth seeking because she did not like anything she saw on the test cards. She created an aura of being a nice person, but managed to make the tester feel guilty for not understanding, and caught in a morass of vagueness and contradiction. Her responses to the TAT seemed to express the basic attitude that all interactions were nonsensical and illogical and "things can be a mess if you don't have your own way.” She became even more disorganized than usual by sexual topics, and she did not seem to differentiate between male and female on the TAT cards. Lidz (1973) contends that the offspring of pathogenic parents is badly prepared for life within the family, and because s/he remains tied to the problems of the prior generation, the child who will become schizophrenic enters adolescence unable to cope with the developmental tasks of achieving independence from his/her parents and gaining an ego identity. Karon and Rosberg (1958) have reconstructed a pathological relationship from material presented in therapy sessions and direct observation of a mother and son's interaction. The mother, who seemed intelligent, likeable, and concerned about her paranoid schizophrenic son when she spoke of him, was nonetheless very damaging to his ego functioning when she interacted with him. On the basis of this and other cases, Karon and Rosberg further clarified the concept of pathogenesis. Karon asserts that the pathogenic mother feels 6 inadequate and compensates for her inadequacy by making demands on the child in terms of her own pathological needs without regard for the welfare of the child, whenever her needs conflict with his. In many respects, the underlying problems of the pathogenic mother of a schizophrenic are similar to those of the schizophrenic offspring, but her defense against her anxieties is damaging to the child (Meyer & Karon, 1967). The pathogenic individual is defined as one who is psychologically destructive when in the dominant position in a dependence relationship in which the needs of the two individuals conflict. The pathogenic individual will indirectly satisfy his/her needs without regard for the needs of the dependent person. For example, this may be accomplished by (1) manipulating (without acknowledging the manipulation) the dependent person to satisfy one's own needs rather than the other's needs, (2) ignoring the expressed needs and wishes of the other, (3) not allowing the other to express his/her negative feelings when those needs are frustrated, (4) or implying that the other is evil for having negative feelings at all. The motivations behind such interactions may be conscious or unconscious; often they are unconscious and accompanied by overt statements expressing a concern about the well-being of the dependent individual (VandenBos & Karon, 1971) . The Pathogenesis Index (PI), a scale used for scoring TAT themes, was derived from the above concept of the pathogenic mother of a schizophrenic patient. The index of pathogenesis has significantly differentiated mothers of schizophrenic children from mothers of normal children (Karon, 1963; Meyer & Karon, 1967; Mitchell, 1968, 1969a). Fathers of schizophrenics scored higher in pathogenesis than fathers of normals; however, the difference did not reach statistical significance 7 (Mitchell, 1969a) . The PI has also been shown to differentiate mothers of severely disturbed schizophrenics from mothers of less disturbed schizophrenics (Nicoles, 1970). Ebrploratory research has found that parents of delinquents have pathogenesis ratings which fall between the ratings of parents of normals and parents of schizophrenics (Mitchell, 1971). Furthermore, mothers who physically abuse their children score significantly higher on the PI than controls (Melnick & Hurley, 1969), and research has suggested that there is somewhat less responsivity to children's needs among working-class mothers of schizophrenic children than among upper- or middle-class mothers of equally disturbed children (Mitchell, 1969b) . Unlike previous research on pathogenesis, the present study uses a nonclinical sample at an earlier stage of development. It is theorized that parental pathogenesis or lack of it is a fairly stable characteristic, unless some structural reorganization of the personality takes place. The present research postulates that children of pathogenic parents will show signs of poor ego functioning which will be evident on the Rorschach. Research (Albee et a1., 1963; Albee et a1., 1964) indicates that those who will become schizophrenic perform much below average while school children. Albee relates this premorbid intellectual deficit to a long-term schizophrenic process present during the childhood of adult schizophrenics, many years before these individuals are recognized and certainly before they are diagnosed as schizophrenic. Intellectual functioning is one aspect of the child's ego functioning which the present study will relate to parental pathogenesis. E E I' . . ll £1.13 This investigation relates parental pathogenesis as determined by the PI to the offspring's ego functioning. Perhaps the clearest definition of ego is that of Guntrip. ”The team 'ego' stands for the 'whole' of the individual's nature and self-knowledge as a 'person,‘ an 'I' in personal relations with other 'I's', whose wholeness can be 'split' by t00>disturbing experiences" (Guntrip, 1969). He further contends that the achievement of a solid ego formation, or the feeling that one is a real person, can only be done in the medium of personal object-relationships. ‘When parents work off their own fears and tensions on the children, the children grow up frightened at heart; the ego remains undeveloped and weak, and deep maturing of personality comes to a standstill. The basic dilemma of the child of “pathogenic" parents is that s/he was born into a situation in which s/he was unable to lay the foundation of a strong ego development, and the child grows up feeling inadequate to the demands of living, being ineffective interpersonally, and full of fear. This early interference in ego development persists because once the initial failure has occurred in laying the foundations of a nonanxious and active self in infancy, a mental organization seems to come into being which blocks the possibility of further deep—level emotional growth. The baby must grow up to become a capable adult, strong enough to look after him/herself and make his/her own contribution to life among other people, but feeling inadequate, gets involved in the process of developing a facade to face life. As time goes by, fears of egoebreakdown may come to be even greater than the original fears of outer reality (Guntrip, 1969). Guntrip contends that a profound sense of weakness and inadequacy does break through into consciousness, but when this occurs, it is because the defenses have cracked, defenses which have been built up against the underlying sense of weakness, to prevent its invasion of consciousness. The obsessional character is an example of a rigid and unyielding pattern of absolute self-control organized with no weakness shown; though the "false self” may hide the infantile nature within. It is the basically infantile nature of the self, which seeks gratification and when frustrated has the capacity to fight yet is fear-ridden, which interferes with further ego development, and this is a deeper problen than that portion of the feeling of weakness that seeps through at various times into everyday consciousness. Poor ego development is basic and causal for all kinds of personality disorder (Guntrip, 1969). ace the child is badly disturbed, and developed enough to realize that s/he is too weak to alter the environment, s/he feels driven to attempt the only other thing possible, namely to alter him/herself in such a way that s/he is no longer conscious of feeling so frightened and weak. The child comes to dislike the infantile part of him/herself and directs a hostile part of the self to crush out the frightened, needy, and therefore, dependent child. However, the crushed child is the primary, natural self and repression of this progressively leads to self-exhaustion and personality disorder. Both repression of, and withdrawal by, the poorly developed ego prevent further normal development of the basic natural self. But schizoid withdrawal into an inner world seems ultimately a more important cause of ego weakness than repression, since it may be a more radical process. That is, the fear-dictated retreat of which 10 Winnicott speaks (Guntrip, 1969) may be continued into an attempt even to escape from internal bad objects. Mahler's (1975) observational studies of mothers and young children suggest pathogenic styles of relating to the child begin early and that there may be crucial periods of vulnerability for the child. Mahler contends that much of our understanding of health and pathology depend on developmental aspects, the most important of which, may be the qualitative assessment of residues of the symbiotic as well as of the separation—individuation periods. She suggests that an old, partially unresolved sense of self-identity and of body boundaries, or old conflicts over separation and separateness, can be reactivated (or can remain peripherally or even centrally active) at any stage of life. Freud asserted that a lifelong although diminishing, emotional dependence on the mother is a universal truth of human existence. The biological unpreparedness of the human infant to maintain life separately makes necessary the prolonged phase of closeness which has been designated "the mother-infant symbiosis" (Mahler, 1975). Mahler's observations suggest that it is from the symbiotic phase of the mother-infant dual unity that those experiential precursors of individual beginnings are derived, which together with inborn constitutional factors, determine every individual's unique somatic and psychological makeup. Thus, mental health, as well as pathology, may be determined by (1) the individual's endowment, (2) the early mother-child interaction and relationship, and (3) crucial events in the child's growing up process--in other words, by positive and negative experiential factors, which impinge upon the pliable makeup of the child's individuating psyche (Mahler, 1975). ll E.J. Anthony (1971) recognized the pertinence of Kierkegaard's insights into the human child's need for his/her mother's emotional support at points when the child experiences maturational spurts. He cites the following passages to illustrate that "the influence of a disturbed and disturbing mother on the individuation of her child is in sharp contrast to that of the ordinary 'good enough' mother" (p. 262): The loving mother teaches her child to walk alone. She is far enough from him so that she cannot actually support him, but she holds out her arms to him. . . .And yet, she does more. Her face beckons like a reward, an encouragement. Thus, the child walks alone with his eyes fixed on his mother's face, not on the difficulties in his way. He supports himself by the arms that do not hold him and constantly strives towards the refuge in his mother's embrace, little suspecting that in the very same moment that he is emphasizing his need of her, he is proving that he can do without her, because he is walking alone (Kierkegaard, 1846, p. 85). But in the other mother it is very different: There is no beckoning encouragement, no blessing at the end of the walk. There is the same wish to teach the child to walk alone, but not as a loving mother does it. For now there is fear that envelopes the child. It weighs him down so that he cannot move forward. There is the same wish to lead him to the goal, but the goal becomes suddenly terrifying (Kierkegaard, 1846, p. 85). This passage suggests the pathogenic mother's fearfulness, ambivalence, unconscious hostility, and need to encapsulate and hinder the child 12 from stepping off on his own. Thus, the pathogenic mother fills these moments of development, when the child feels a pull for separation and a need to assert individuation, with apprehension, so that the child not only has nowhere to go, but s/he is afraid to get anywhere (Anthony, 1971). Mahler (1975) refers to her 1958 publications in which she hypothesizes that normal separation-individuation is the first crucial prerequisite for the development and maintenance of the "sense of identity.“ Concern with the problem of identity arose from observing a puzzling clinical phenomenon, namely, that the psychotic child never attains a feeling of wholeness, of individual entity, let alone "a sense of human identity." It was thought that something had gone astray in the very earliest interactions within the mother-infant unit, leading to either a deanimated, frozen wall between the subject and the human object, or a fusion, melting, and lack of differentiation between the self and the nonself-a corplete blurring of boundaries. Mahler (1975) found, in observational studies, that those infants whose mothers enjoyed the symbiotic phase without too much conflict, those infants who were saturated, but not oversaturated, during this period of important oneness with the mother, seemed to start at the average time to show signs of active differentiation by distancing slightly from the mother's body. (laservations suggested that it is important that the mother as protective shield gradually recede, so as not to hinder the individuating ego's gradual exercise of autonomy. Mahler (1975) reports that some parents could not accept the child's demandingness; others, by contrast, were unable to face the child's gradual separation—the fact that the child was becoming 13 increasingly independent of and separate from the parent. In some cases in which the mother acted clearly out of her own symbiotic-parasitic need, rather than out of regard for the infant, differentiation set in almost vehemently. This happened, for example, in the case of a little boy, as early as 4 or 5 months of age because his mother was symbiotically too enveloping. A strikingly different transition from the symbiotic to the separation-individuation phase was observed in another little boy who had a close and prolonged symbiotic relationship with the mother. Both of this child's parents had symbiotic-parasitic needs and overvalued their child as a vegetative being, prolonging the symbiotic dependency (Parens & Saul, 1971). This clearly slowed down the child's libidinal investment in his motor functions. Those children, in Mahler's (1975) investigation, who had the greatest difficulty establishing a workable distance between themselves and their mothers were those whose mothers became ambivalent toward them as soon as they ceased to be part of them—their symbiotic child. At times the mothers seemed to avoid close body contact; at other times, they might interrupt their child in his/her autonomous activities to pick up, hug, or hold their child. The mothers did this when they needed it, not when the child did. This lack of empathy on the mothers part seered to make it difficult for the child to functioi at a distance from her. Mahler's observations indicate that the child's desire for expanded autonomy leads to an active extension of the mother-child world: primarily to include father. Father, as a love object, from very early on, belongs to an entirely different category of love objects from mother, that is, although he is not fully outside the 14 symbiotic union, neither is he ever fully part of it (Abelin, 1971). Significant for this study is the observation (Mahler, 1975) that in certain cases the effects of a child's less than optimal relationship with the mother were ameliorated by a good relationship and subsequent identification with the father. Masterson's (1976) case studies, however, suggest that more typically the child whose growth is already heavily burdened by the mother's regressive pull does not get "the average expectable father" who influences him/her toward growth and reality, and who forms a "bridge" out of the symbiotic mother-infant unit. Although the specifics vary with each case, the father's influence almost always is toward a reinforcement of the mother-child exclusive or clinging relationship, rather than either opposing or making efforts to lead the child away from it. The main feature is that he is typically not available to the child as an uncontaminated object to support the forces of individuation and mastery of reality. Mahler (1975) stresses the importance of separation, individuation and mastery of reality during the early years of childhood. Similarly, Masterson (1976) points out that at prepuberty, a challenge to the child's defensive system usually occurs, when a second marked develOpmental maturation of the ego occurs, creating a period of special vulnerability. This growth spurt, manifested by a thrust toward activity colbined with a turn toward reality, is similar in scope to the maturation of the ego that occurred in the separation-individuation phase. This maturation, together with the need to separate further from the mother, produces a recapitulation of that early phase of development, that is, a second separation—individuation phase (Masterson, 1976) . 15 Deutsch (1944) suggests that prepuberty is a phase in which sexual instincts are weakest and development of the ego is most intense, and that the phase is characterized by a thrust of activity and turning toward growth and independence; thus, it is an intensive process of adaptation to reality and mastery of the environment. The adolescent is caught between the past and the future, between childhood and adulthood, just as the infant is caught between a symbiotic relationship and autonomy. Deutsch emphasizes that the struggle for independence in this period is strongly reminiscent of the processes that take place approximately between the age 18 months and 3 years, that is, the transition from the symbiotic stage to autonomy. Masterson (1976) has found that the borderline syndrome often makes its clinical appearance during this time; whereas, somewhat later, at age 15, the high-risk period for schizophrenia begins (Mednick & Schulsinger, 1970). E . | . I | The present study relates parental pathogenesis to the daughter's ego functioning by means of their scores on projective tests (see Appendix A for an explanation of the term projection). Projectives offer the advantage of providing data which consist not only of the conscious attitudes or what the individuals feel ought to be their conscious attitudes, but data which reflect a motivational pattern, conscious or unconscious. This is particularly important for an investigation of these variables, since parental pathogenesis may involve subtle destructive behaviors with an overt display of concern, and poor ego development may be camouflaged by a "false self.” Projective testing is generally concerned with an assessment of the ego, ego strength and defenses, assets and liabilities (Rabin, 16 1960). Bolgar states ". . .that the proper field of investigation of projective psychology is character, its formation and its manifestations, and that the theoretical emphasis, insofar as it is psychoanalytic, should be the psychology of the ego and not the psychology of the id" (in Klopfer et a1., 1956). However, there is a difference of opinion regarding the latter part of the statement. Holt (1956), for instance, is concerned with the “gauging of primary processes" as well as the secondary processes of the ego. Rabin (1960) suggests that it may be a matter of emphasis, for it is difficult to evaluate the ego without reflecting certain implications with respect to the id. Paradoxically, in order to penetrate the depths of character formation, of the ego and its defenses, in projective testing, an important prerequisite is the relaxation of some of the controls and the partial, voluntary surrender of the ego's secondary process (Rabin, 1960). That is, in the projective test situation, as in states of artistic creativity, a person is expected to "make believe" or imagine, to tell what a stimulus looks like or to tell a story, rather than to give a factual description of the picture or some other stimulus to which s/he is reacting. Naturally, people vary to the extent that they are able to “regress in the service of the ego" (Schafer, 1954) in cooperation with the examiner's request. Rabin (1960) contends that the degree of freedom with which this operation is accorplished is, in itself, an important index of the ego's freedom from threat, that is, the freedom of the individual to allow some regression to becote manifest during the testing situation. The above argument suggests that Rorschach R is a sign of good ego functioning. 17 The Rorschach has been used successfully to assess ego functioning. In a longitudinal study, Glatt and Karon (1974) found that as schizophrenic patients were rated as improved, Rorschach appropriate pure form responses w , human moveient responses (LI) , and number of responses (3), increased, while Perseveration decreased. Clinically improved subjects became more adaptable (increased 3) , flexible (decreased Perseveration) , showed better reality testing (increased E13), and were better able to delay impulse discharge and to utilize fantasy for the expression of inner drives (increased )1) . I! ll' EllE lllL'lJ W. The 2+3. represents the proportion of Pure Form responses which have been scored either superior (+) or ordinary (0) regarding the use of form. In the Exner system (1974) form is graded on a four point scale to differentiate levels of form adequacy. Superior (+) form quality refers to the unusually well developed and articulated use of form that enriches the quality of the percept without sacrificing the appropriateness of form involved. The ordinary (0) form quality answer involves the obvious, easily developed use of form, wherein the content and blot areas are congruent. The two categories of poor form quality involve the use of form where the percept is not easily perceived and the situation where the answer is imposed on the blot area with total, or near total disregard for the structure of the area. Form quality is one of the most important elements among the structural data of the Rorschach (Exner, 1974). Generally, the inclusion of form in a response has been considered as an "ego" or thinking operation. According to Rapaport (1946), the use of form appears to represent the operations of perception and reasoning. It 18 reflects the ability of the subject to direct his/her attention ard ideation to the elelents of control, discriminating judgment, and regard for the standards of the environment. Beck (1945) holds that, where the quality of the form use is "good" (+ or o), the subject demonstrates a respect for reality, whereas the frequent use of "poor" form quality manifests a disregard for this element. The work of Ames (1971) indicates that while the £13 is generally low for the very young child, it typically exceeds 80% by the sixth year, and her adolescent groups routinely yield mean Eflls in excess of 90. The most striking data concerning form quality, however, have developed in studies of the more seriously disturbed psychiatric patients, schizophrenics in particular. As Weiner points out, "Virtually all studies of iii. in schizophrenia and control groups have replicated the findings presented by Beck and Rickers-Ovsiankina in their historically significant 1938 contributions." In each of those studies, schizophrenics showed a mean £11 in the 60's while controls yielded a mean :33 significantly higher, 87.3 for the Rickers group, 83.9 for the Beck group (Weiner, 1966). In addition, the m reasonably indicates the subject's capacity to deal more effectively with stresses. Goldberger (1961), using an isolation study design, found that subjects manifesting a greater “reality testing strength," as indicated by the adequacy of good form quality, were more capable of handling ”the primary process intrusions of sensory deprivations" than were subjects yielding a lower quality of form answers. There was, however, one caution regarding the use of Eli in this study. Ordinarily, some "bending" of the reality features of the cards is expected. Thus if the E31 were very high (near 100%), the interpreter would have to consider the possibility of 19 an ”undue" preoccupation with reality that may cause the subject sore sacrifice of uniqueness (Earner, 1974). In the extremely high ranges, B and M, as well as £31, may be associated with psychopathology. The study of Glatt and Karon (1974) suggests a monotonic relationship exists, with increases in £13, 3, and M being associated with improvement in their schizophrenic patients. However in a population which is not specifically schizophrenic, variance is likely to be greater for the offspring with poorer ego functioning, representing various pathological adaptations to the parental pathogenesis. W. As an indication of productivity level, the total number of Rorschach responses, 3 reflects ability to respond to a task (taking the Rorschach) which requires a measure of ingenuity in a novel situation, a flexibility of attitudes, tolerance of ambiguity, and an expenditure of effort (Neiger, Sleman, & Quirk, 1965). Mastery and cotpetence and adaptive regression in the service of the ego are the principal ego functions expressed in 3. Studies have consistently shown an increase in B from early childhood to early adulthood (Ames, 1966; Beck, Beck, Levitt, & Molish, 1961; Hermendinger, 1953; Ledwith, 1959; Meile-Dworetzki, 1956; Rabin & Beck, 1950; Thetford et a1., 1951). Through faulty design, previous research has, at times, confused a change in productivity (B) with a change in the value of other scores (Glatt, 1971). Piotrowski and Schreiber (1952) offer an example of this problem. Patients getting psychoanalytic therapy demonstrated considerable Rorschach change upon retest, especially in human movement and color. No significant changes occurred among individuals getting a more superficial, supportive therapy. Moreover, 20 the psychoanalytic therapy group averaged a nearly two—fold increase in 3 on the posttherapy Rorschach, while the other group's average productivity did not change. Since 3 correlates with most other scores (Cronbach, 1949; Fiske & Baughman, 1953), the increases made by the psychoanalytic therapy subjects may have been the result of becoming more productive. Fiske and Baughman (1953) published a table of the average values observed for each common Rorschach score at varying levels of B. When each mean score value in Piotrowski and Schreiber's psychoanalytic therapy group was corpared to its "expected" value at the corresponding level of R (an admittedly crude procedure necessitated by the availability of group, and not of individual, data) it can be seen that the statistically significant increase in C-sum upon retest (from 4.8 to 7.5) was of the magnitude to be expected when 3 increases to the extent that it did (from 41.6 to 76.8) . Not all the score increases, however, could be explained as mere artifacts of increases in B. Gains in EC and M were disproportionately high when cotpared to their predicted values at the higher 3 level (Glatt, 1971) . The present study partials out the effects of B, as explained in the results section; however, by using Exner's system, which does not encourage additional responses, the R was not expected to vary to the extent that it has in research using other systems. W. The human movetent response (M) is scored when human activity, of a passive or active nature, is perceived and the movetent has been articulated for a response. Probably M has been the subject of more investigation than any other Rorschach determinant. The primary problem in the researching of M is that the category of M includes several different kinds of responses. For 21 example, M's occurring to tiny areas appear to be different from M's occurring to the whole blot (K) or commonly used detail ()2) areas in the type of psychological activity involved (Exner, 1974). There are probably also differences in psychological activity when M's include only one human figure than when they involve»more than one human figure. The Mfs involving aggressive content are somewhat different than M's which convey a sense of cooperativeness, and so on. In spite of this, the large quantity of research which has been corpleted concerning M does establish an understanding of the basic psychological process involved in the formation of the M answer (EXner, 1974). Rorschach offered the postulate that M represents an "internalization" phenomenon. He believed that M manifests the more deliberate inner experience in a.manner which is also affectively adaptive. Beck (1945, 1967) has used this Rorschach postulate as a reflection of emotions which are in some way concealed from an overt display. The nucleus of his position is that M answers indicate an awareness of the external world, where the vestiture of energy is used to develop a private fantasy through which a displacement of pent-up needs is permitted. Klopfer (1954) and Hertz (1951) have both suggested that M should be interpreted as a psychological process in which a functional relationship exists between the fantasy life of the individual and the external orientations to reality and object relations. Thus Beck, Klopfer, and Hertz all tend to agree that M does represent some form of internalization. Rapaport (1946) admits to only a partial acceptance of Rorschach's positim on M. He agrees with Beck that M does represent a readiness to make a response, maintaining that in sore instances this readiness will result in 22 internalization, while in other instances the product will be an overt gesture. Thus, Rapaport rejects the notion of introversion as such. For him, the M answer is more indicative of a type of delay of response than a tendency toward internalization, the delay being provoked by the more sophisticated cognitive functions so as to permit a more consciously determined response to occur. That is, Rapaport's position is that M reflects a control of spontaneous behavior, which may result in either internalization or externalization. Piotrowski's position concerning the interpretation of M responses differs most from the previously mentioned positions. He contends that the correlation between M and overt behavior is positive: "The M indicates prototypal roles in life, i.e., definite tendencies, deeply erbedded in the subject and not easily modified, to assume repeatedly the same attitude or attitudes in dealing with others when matters felt to be important and personal are involved" (Exner, 1974). While this position is different from the other systematizers, there is at least a segment of agreement between Klopfer, Hertz, and Piotrowski in that all regard M as bridging the inner resources with the external reality. There is also some segment of agreerent between the Piotrowski and Rapaport positions in that both concede, Rapaport directly and Piotrowski indirectly, that M may represent an internalization action by the subject, and both agree that the type of internal reasoning manifest in the M response is a form of inhibition of impulse, which may be transient (Exner, 1974). Numerous studies (Paulsen, 1941; Abrams, 1955; Altus, 1958; Somer, 1958; Tanaka, 1958; Ogdon & Allee, 1959) have established that M has a direct relationship to intellectual operations, by demonstrating a positive correlation between 1.0., or some other 23 direct measure of intelligence, and the frequency and/or quality of M responses. Ames (1971) reports that the frequency of M is very low in the young child, and that a steady increase in M frequency occurs to about the 10th year, and Kallstedt (1952) found significantly less M in the protocols of adolescents than in those of adults. However, according to one study (Hertz, 1943) more of the prepubescent 12 year old girls scored higher in M than girls who were more physically mature (pubescent 12—year-olds) . The Hertz data may reflect an intensification of inner living during the year before the pubertal crisis, which is followed by a decrease with the onset of pubescence. Schulman (1953) reports that M is positively correlated with abstract thinking and has demonstrated that the activity involved in both functions requires some delaying operations. Levine, Glass, and Melzoff (1959) have also demonstrated that M and the higher levels of intellectual operation require delaying activities. Piotrowski (1939), Halpern (1940), and Stotsky (1952) have reported significant increases in the frequency of M among patients who show improvement versus those who are unimproved. In a relapse study, Exner (1974) found that the admission and postdischarge Rorschachs of 71 schizophrenics did not reveal a significant increase in M frequency although the quality of the M's was better. However, when the protocols of the 19 subjects who relapsed during the first year after discharge were corpared with those of the 52 nonrelapsers, it was found that the nonrelapsers had given significantly more M's in both their admission and postdischarge records. It is difficult to summarize the full psychological meaning of M responses since complex activities seem to be involved. The intellectual base includes a sort of reasoning, the corponents of 24 imagination, and a form of higher level conceptualization (Exner, 1974) . Exner further describes M as a form of delay from yielding to more spontaneous impulses, which depends on an active kind of ideation. It seems to reflect a "deliberate” inner experience which manifests fantasies that are related to the external world. The inner experiences, or fantasies, seem to play an important role in formulating the response selected for a given stimulus. That is, the M response reflects the ability to thwart and/or displace response tendencies into a continuing ideational activity, or to externalize, either directly or indirectly in behaviors which are interpersonally related. Exner (1974) contends that the process does not appear to be conscious, but rather a form of cautious defensiveness through which responses to the world are sorted through. According to Exner (1974) M cannot be interpreted very accurately without also reviewing other variables, especially the responsiveness to color. Rorschach recognized this in his development of the Experience Balance which represents the ratio of the total number of human movement determinants to the weighted sum of the color determinants. The present study also considers M in relation to other Rorschach variables in the epAEA ratio. W The experience potential (22) is a derivation obtained by adding together all the animal movetent (EM), inanimated movement (m), shading (I). texture (I), vista (2), and achromatic color (g1) determinants. The experience actual (EA) represents the sum of the human movement responses (M) and weighted sum of color ((2) responses. The sort of experience or activity expressed in the EA is organized, that is, available to the individual. The painful affects and the more unorganized needs, as 25 represented in the grey-black features of the blots and in EM and m answers, are not organized. On the contrary, these features of the .ep work on the individual rather than for him/her. The data support the notion that the EA and :9 are relatively stable over time unless some dramatic alteration of the basic organization of affects and inner experience takes place (Exner, 1974). Exner indicates that the EA reflects the full volume of the organized activity available to the individual. Three studies were corpleted to test this concept. In the first, two groups of 30 each, patients and nonpatients, were retested after an 18 month interval. The mean BA for the 30 controls at the first administration of the test was 6.25, and 6.75 at the retest. The patient group was subdivided into two groups on the basis of independent ratings from both professionals, in the blind, and relatives regarding the effectiveness of treatment. Eighteen subjects were judged as improved and 12 as unimproved. The mean BA for the improved group was 3.75 at pretreatment and 7.25 at posttreatrent. The mean He‘s for the unimproved group were 3.50 at pretreatment and 4.25 at posttreatment. The difference between the groups at posttreatment is significant at the .05 level. The second study investigated reliability and used three groups of 15 subjects, each selected from a nonpatient sample and retested twice at 9 and 18 month intervals. Correlations from the m's of the three groups calculated between the first and second testings are .88, .91, and .81, and between the first and third testings are .92, .89, and .78. In the third study, two groups of 12 patients each were tested prior to and after the termination of treatient. One group was 26 treated with a dynamic, uncovering form of psychotherapy, whereas the second group was treated using supportive and directive methods plus various forms of environmental manipulation. Both groups were retested 10 weeks after treatient had terminated. The mean EA's were essentially the same at the pretreatment testing, 4.50 for the uncovering treatment group, and 4.75 for the supportive group. The posttreatment EA's were significantly different, showing a mean for the dynamic treatment group of 8.25, and 5.50 for the supportive group. These results suggest that when some reconstructive impact occurs to the individual, s/he is able to organize more resources. The data also support the notion that the EA is relatively stable over time unless some alteration of the basic organization of affects and inner experience takes place (Exner, 1974) . When 30 controls were corpared to improved and unimproved patients, 22 of the 30 controls had 22's that were less than the EA in the first testing. In the 18 month retest, the :2 was less than the EA for 23 of these 30 subjects. The ep;EA difference for the 30 psychiatric subjects at the first testing showed that 21 of the 30 had gp's which were greater than the EA. In the 18 month posttesting the egEA difference had shifted for 15 subjects from a greater ep than EA to a greater EA than :32. It was found that 14 of the 18 subjects rated as improved have ep's greater than the EA in the first test and less than the EA in the retest. In another study, the posttreatrent records show that all of the subjects treated with dynamic therapy had EA's greater than ep's. In fact, in half of those cases the EA exceeded the ep by at least three times the frequency. The above research suggests that as subjects "improve" the ezEA difference shifts from a greater :2 than EA to a greater EA than ep, 27 while those subjects rated as unimproved, show no essential change in either ep or E after treatment. Exner (1974) draws the conclusion that some, or all of the unorganized or uncontrolled activity, may have become organized and made available to those subjects rated as improved, as the EA's increased significantly and the 32's decreased significantly. On the other hand, he notes that a more conservative conclusion is that many of those stresses giving rise to EM, m, and shading answers were alleviated, thus causing either a reduction in ep, or an increase in EA, or both. The way in which the psychological resources are utilized appears to become altered. For exarple, in some cases where Pure 2 increases with improvement, tactics of delay and affective control have been used so that resources may be expended more conservatively. In other instances, increases in EA may be due to greater control and organization of resources at the higher levels of operation. However, in either event, Exner (1974) contends it is an alteration of the activities reflected in the 22 that permits these changes to occur. That is, greater controls are developed to contend with the forces that act on the individual, either neutralizing them to a reasonable extent, or reorganizing them in such a way that they act for the individual. W Exner's (1974) Special Scorings for disordered thinking reflect impairment to the cognitive operations. Perservation (25y) reflects a kind of cognitive inflexibility. Deviant verbalizations (121) are characterized by distorted language usage which impede the subject's ability to communicate clearly. Incongruous cotbinations (M) consist of the condensation of separate blot details into a single incongruous 28 percept. Fabulized cotbinations (ream) are scored when an implausible relationship is posited between two or more blot details. Contamination (52mm refers to two or more impressions of a single blot area which are fused into a single percept that clearly violates reality. Autistic logic (AIME) reflects a form of loose or circumstantial thinking. These scorings occur most commonly among schizophrenic subjects, although they have been shown to occur in other disorders and nonpatients to a lesser degree (Exner & Weiner, 1978) . The thinking and reasoning difficulties reflected in the special scorings is best considered in a developmental context. Responses containing features that would be assigned a special score are quite common among preschool children and are not considered indicative of psychological disorders. Such responses gradually decrease as cognitive maturation gradually approaches its adult level (Exner, Weiner, s. Schuyler, 1978). In a normative study of 1,570 Rorschach protocols, the nonpatient adolescents were significantly more likely than the nonpatient adults to include one or more Aim, mm. and Em responses in their protocols. 121 showed a nonsignificant trend in this direction, whereas comm did not appear in either group. Despite the relative frequency of these indices of disordered thinking in the records of the nonpatient adolescents as coipared to the nonpatient adults, the nonpatient adolescents were still significantly less likely than the behavior problem and the withdrawn adolescents to give m, Aux-1, m, and m responses. The latter two groups were about equally likely to give such responses. m showed a nonsignificant tendency to discriminate among the groups and was given only by the withdrawn adolescents. The nonpatient adult, outpatient adult, and 29 inpatient schizophrenic adult groups showed a respectively increasing frequency of giving or. mm. mm. mecca. and roman responses. and the differences among the groups were statistically significant for each of the five indices (Exner & Weiner, 1978). All of these special scorings reflect patterns of responding in which subjects fail to maintain a legical or realistic way of dealing with their experiences. When they occur, they may offer an important clue to impaired functioning which may be a transient phenomenon, or may be a manifestation of a more subtle or incipient disturbance, or may represent a gross psychological disarray. Generally, Exner, et a1. (1978) consider 9mm and AME answers to be a reflection of more extreme distortions of reality than 21, m EABCSM, and Efl (see Appendix B for scoring criteria). WW. Developmental quality (129) refers to the quality of responses to the various locations of the blots. In particular, 29 refers to the level of organization, integration, imagination, and accuracy of perception. A synthesized response (+) is scored when "unitary or discrete portions of the blot are perceptually articulated and integrated or cotbined into a single percept." The ordinary response (0) is scored when "a discrete area of the blot is selected and articulated so as to eiphasize the gross outline and obvious structural features of the area selected." A response is scored vague (1) when "a diffuse or general impression is offered to the blot or blot area in a manner which avoids the necessity of articulation of specific outlines or structural features." An arbitrary (-) response is scored when ”articulation of the blot or blot area is inconsistent with the structural limitations of the blot” (Earner, 1974). 30 Developmental quality scores represent an area of the Rorschach which has been researched with considerable success in that the findings are very consistent. Goldfried, Stricker, and Weiner (1971) conclude that this approach differentiates children at the various age levels, and also provides a basis from which normal, neurotics, brain-damaged, and schizophrenics can be differentiated. Generally, the higher m scores (+ and o) are found among the brighter and/or more psychologically "effective" subjects, while the lower 129. scores (v and —) are noted among very young children, intellectually limited subjects, brain-damaged, and the severely disturbed. In addition, studies (Earner, 1974) have shown that lower 1;} scores are related to tendencies toward motoric expression of affect, and that subjects manifesting such scores have less "capacity" for delay of impulse. Becker (1956) found process schizophrenics yielding greater numbers of lower m scores than reactive schizophrenics. 'Exner (1974) describes the similar findings of Wilensky when he compared ”closed-ward" to "open-ward” schizophrenics. And Friedman (1952) demonstrated that hebephrenic and catatonic schizophrenics manifest substantial quantities of lower 29 scores and suggested that this may be interpreted as characterizing a primative, rigid, diffuse sort of cognition. The reports of Siegal (1953) suggest that the paranoid schizophrenic, while still manifesting high frequencies of low :33 scores, tends to function cognitively at a higher level than do catatonics or hebephrenics. Exner points out that protocols of nonpsychiatric subjects frequently contain 129 location selections which have scores of y, and in some instances, even ;. However, where low m scores are proportionally abundant, the interpreter must consider the possibility 31 of a rigid, concrete, and somewhat immature kind of cognition, which may be confirmed by other data in the record. At the other extreme, when a record contains a large number of high 29 scores, the flexibility and seeming higher maturational level of the cognitive operations are suggested (Exner, 1974). W. The present study utilized Elizur's content scoring for hostility, which has been used more often than other Rorschach approaches and, by and large, these other scorings represent modifications or elaborations of Elizur's system (Goldfried et a1., 1971) . In his description of what the content scoring of the Rorschach purports to measure, Elizur (1949) uses the term hostility to refer to "feelings of resentment and enmity, which are often repressed in our culture, but almost inevitably show up in the individual's distorted attitudes toward people, either being too antagonistic or too submissive" (see Appendix C for Elizur's Hostility Scoring System). Interscorer agreement on total hostility level is high (Cummings, 1954; Elizur, 1949; Forsyth, 1959; Sanders & Cleveland, 1953; Siegel, 1956; Smith & Coleman, 1956) . The correlations between two scorers were .98 (Sanders 8 Cleveland, 1953), .92 (Cummings, 1954), and .78 (Siegel, 1956). Smith and Coleman (1956) obtained an intraclass correlation of .90 for three scorers. Based on their analysis of research using the system, Goldfried and his colleagues (1971) coiclude that the interscorer agreerent is good with the average correlation coefficient being sorewhere in the neighborhood of .90. The data available for delinquent and nondelinquent adolescents (60er et a1., 1952), and nailbiting children (Cummings, 1954) are relevant. The Rorschach records of 13 adolescent delinquents were cotpared with those of 13 nondelinquents matched on the basis of age, 32 I.Q., and socio—economic background. The delinquents were randomly selected from court files on the basis of the “antisocial" acts which they had committed. The results indicated that despite the lower total B, the delinquents obtained significantly higher 1m scores. The mean for the delinquent group was 10.0 and the mean for the noidelinquent group was 4.0. The group of nailbiters described by Cummings (1954) had a mean EL score of 11.93. recent research using Elizur's Hostility Scoring (Crain & Smoke, 1981) suggests that greater emphasis should be placed on specific forms of aggressive content. Using a group of 42 children who had been referred for psychological evaluation, matched with 42 children who were considered well-aijusted, they found that the normal children more frequently focused on aggression in the context of direct interactions (fights); whereas the problematic children talked as if the figures were at the mercy of unknown or unreal attackers. In the normal children's aggressive responses combatants usually seeied fairly equal in power and the outcome seeied at least partly under the figure's control. In cmtrast, the problematic children's responses were marked by an objectless overwhelming quality. They frequently talked about the victims of violence without saying who the aggressor was, and when they did specify the aggressor, it often was a frightening overpowering moister or a creature that might eat one up. (Elizur's Hostility Scoring presented in Appendix C is modified according to this research.) W. Similar to Elizur's system of scoring for hostility, his system of content scoring for anxiety has been the most popular Rorschach measure of anxiety with researchers (Goldfried, et a1., 1971). Elizur's definition of the construct his system purports 33 to measure falls more in line with what is usually termed "anxiety“ rather than “fear" or "guilt." He implied that this condition of high drive or arousal has an internal rather than an external basis. Further, the conceptualization of anxiety reflected in the scoring system refers to experienced anxiety, rather than "unconscious anxiety," that is, the anxiety which has been successfully reduced or avoided by means of defensive maneuvers (Elizur, 1949) . (See Appendix D for Elizur's Anxiety Scoring System.) Just as the scoring criteria for Rorschach anxiety level parallel the hostility scoring system, so is there a cotparably high interscorer agreement for this system as well (Cummings, 1954; Elizur, 1949; Forsyth, 1959; Mogar, 1962; Sanders & Cleveland, 1953). Elizur (1949) using a group of neurotic subjects comparable to normal subjects on the basis of age, sex, and I.Q., found that the mean AL score for the neurotic grwp was 12.5 and the mean AL score for the normal group was 5.2. High and low anxiety college students, determined by their scores on the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale, scored 9.88 and 6.42, respectively, on Anxiety Level (Westrope, 1953) . Delinquent and nondelinquent adolescents, referred to earlier in the section on hostility level scoring, produced AL scores of 11.38 and 8.15, in that order (Gorlow et a1., 1952); whereas grade-school nailbiters scored 11.93 (Cummings, 1954). In an overview of research in this area, Goldfried and his colleagues (1971) point to some hints of a developmental trend in the salples of normal subjects, with AL scores decreasing from the adolescents to the college students to the adults. More recent research (Aron, 1982) adds further support to the construct validity of Elizur's anxiety score. The study corpared the Rorschach protocols of subjects experiencing high and low life—stress 34 conditions, determined on the basis of their extreme scores on a measure of stressful life events, the College Schedule of Recent Experience (Anderson, 1972). Subjects in the high stress condition responded to the Rorschach with significantly more anxious content (p < .05) than did subjects in the low stress condition. W The purpose of the present investigation is to examine the relationship between parental pathogenesis and the daughters' ego functioning. Theories have been proposed (Mahler, 1975; Masterson, 1976; Lidz, 1965, 1973) which posit a causal relationship between parental pathogenecity and poor ego functioning in the offspring that ranges from difficulties in separating from the parent to schizophrenic breakdown. Previous research with the Rorschach factors adequate form quality (131-3), human movement (M), disordered thinking (DI), proportion of disorganized to organized coping resources (epfiA), proportion of mature developrental quality (21), number of responses (3), anxiety level (AL) , and hostility level (ML) indicate that these factors can provide important information regarding the subjects' ego functioning. The present study investigates the relationship between these Rorschach indices of ego functioning and parental pathogenecity. The observational measure which is the teachers' ratings of the daughters' school performance, in relation to their estimated abilities, provides a further measure of some aspects of the daughters' ego functioning. Signs of poor ego functioning may indicate that these individuals are at greater risk for future severe psychopathology. If future studies confirm that this speculation is valid, then assessment techniques, such as the TAT and Rorschach, which might be able to 35 single out individuals who are highly pathogenic and/or have poor ego functioning, could play a useful part in the prevention of serious psychopathology. Another purpose of the present study is to extend the research on pathogenesis. Previous research on pathogenesis has compared normal to clinical groups and groups with different degrees of psychopathology. The present study extends the research on pathogenesis by investigating pathogenesis within a "normal" sarple, and determining how it relates to several relatively subtle signs of ego functioning. 36 STRHENENT'OF HYPOTHESES Hypothesis 1: There is a negative relationship between level of pathogenesis of parents and ego functioning in the daughters. Cperational predictions: A. There is a positive correlation between the pathogenesis score for the parents and (l) anxiety level score, (2) hostility level score, (3) ratio of epAEA, and (4) the indices of disordered thinking for the daughters. There is a negative correlation between the pathogenesis score for the parents and 131, M, m, and B for the daughters. There is a negative correlation between pathogenesis (PI) for the parents and teachers ratings of the daughters' school performance. The teacher ratings of better school performance correlate positively with the Rorschach indices of better ego functioning for the daughters. Hypothesis 2: The negative relationship of pathogenesis in the 'mothers to the daughters' level of ego functioning is greater than that of the fathers'. Operational prediction: A. The pathogenesis scores for the mothers, in contrast to the fathers, correlate more highly with the indices of ego functioning in the daughters. 37 Low High Banners; W Exner ' s Ind ices 29 High Low 313. High (below 95%) Low (possible variability) 2’1 High Low (possible variability) 3 Medium Low (possible variability or no change) are Low High 122 Low High Elizur's Indices AL Low High EL Low High W Thachers' Ratings High Low 38 lflififlll Sibjscta The subjects were 20 sets of parents and their prepubescent 12-year-old seventh grade daughters. In using a salple of children who attend the same school, are the sate age, sex, have the same pubertal status, and have attained the expected grade level for their age, the study controlled for differences due to normal development and sex, and minimized differences due to social class and intelligence. Socioeconomic status (SIB) was determined by the Hollingshead Index of Social Position (Hollingshead & Redlich, 1958) which is based on a weighted coibination of the father's level of occupation and of education. The daughter's intelligence was assessed by the Thorndike Vocabulary Test (Miner, 1957) a 20-item multiple choice test (see Appendix F). The number answered correctly was the score used in the statistical analysis. Prmedurs The parents were recruited by telephone from the C.E. MacDonald Middle School telephone directory in approximately the following manner: "I am a doctoral candidate in clinical psychology conducting a research project that relates parents' and daughters' scores on projective tests. The projective tests to be used are the Rorschach ink blot test and the Thematic Apperception Test. For this research project, I am looking for biological parents, who 39 both live in the home, and their lZ—year-old prepubertal daughters—prepubertal so that the girls may be as similar as possible in level of maturity. Does your family meet these specifications? Are you interested in becoming involved in this project?” "This would involve coming to Michigan State University, Snyder Hall, room 4. You, your husband (or wife), and your daughter would each be tested individually. This would take approximately 1 1/2 hours for each person. I would also like you to bring to the session your child's most recent report card. The identities of all participants will remain anonymous, and the results of individual testing will be treated with strict confidence. At the coipletion of this project, you will receive a summary of the findings." Subjects were assessed individually in the following order: mother, father, daughter. Each subject was seen for approximately 15 minutes prior to testing in order to allay anxiety, establish minimum rapport, and offer assurances regarding the confidentiality of test responses. During the first fifteen minutes, the mothers were asked about any other children living in the home. The fathers were asked about any other adults living in the home, and the daughters were asked to name the three most important adults to them. The Thorndike Vocabulary Test was also administered to the daughters at this time. The daughters brought with them their most recent report card, from which the teachers' ratings were recorded. The teachers' ratings were an observational measure of the daughters' behavior in school over an extended period of time. The measure was chosen to provide a further test of the relationship between parental pathogenesis and ego 40 functioning in the daughters and to provide an observational validity check on the asseserent instruments. Each daughter was rated by her seven teachers regarding the student's progress in meeting objectives established for her. Note that these ratings were not measures of performance against a standard set for all children at that grade level, but were measures of how closely the students' performances matched their estimated abilities (see Appendix H). W- Fathers and mothers were individually presented with the 20 male and female TAT cards, respectively, by the investigator. Nofiirective questions were used sparingly and only when it appeared that the subject had becore confused in his/her response. Stories were typed on separate sheets of paper with no identification clues and randomized. The daughters were assessed with the Rorschach according to Exner's Comprehensive System (1974). The Rorschach, including free association and inquiry, was administered. Only those instructions and questions and encouragetents recomended by Banner (1974) were used. At no time did the investigator atterpt to facilitate the scoring by questioning the subject. Personal identification data were not included on the Rorschach protocols, which were randomized before being scored. The questim of possible cmtaminatim, since the investigator administered the projectives and was aware of the hypotheses, may be answered as follows. The investigator was not aware of parental pathogenesis scores at the time of the daughters' Rorschach testing. Furthermore, all projective testing utilized a side-by-side seating arrangetent to lessen non-verbal communication, and by using the Exner system (1974) interaction was minimal and additional respoises were 41 not encouraged. Furthermore, interscorer reliability for the judges was adequate. W. The investigator and another doctoral candidate, who had no knowledge of the hypotheses, scored the randomized Rorschach protocols in accordance with the Exner system (1974) and Elizur's content analysis (1949), and the randomized TAT stories according to Karon's PI (Meyer & Karon, 1967) (see Appendices B, C, D, E for further information on scoring). The W was determined from the parents' TAT stories which were classified as (a) pathogenic, (b) benign, or (c) unscoreable. Basically, stories were classified as pathogenic if, when the needs of a dominant individual and a dependent individual conflicted, the dominant individual indirectly satisfied his/her needs by manipulating the dependent individual. However, if the dominant individual acknowledged and/or met the needs of the dependent individual, the story was scored benign. If there was no interaction, the story was judged unscoreable. A pathogenic score was tabulated for each parent using the formula: P/(P-IIB), where P is the number of stories judged pathogenic and B is the number of stories judged benign. In addition, an average pathogenic score was determined for the parents of each child (see Appendix E for the scoring criteria). The W (Fit). calculated from the daughters' Rorschach protocols, measures perceptual accuracy. It is the proportion of pure form answers, which are clearly good in form fit to the area of the blot, to the total number of pure form responses. The W (R) refers to the total number of responses given in a protocol. The number of responses are simply counted. 42 The W (M) refers to the number of responses reflecting human movetent of an active or passive nature, where the moveient has been articulated for the response. The M is scored for both whole or partial human figures. The total number of such responses are counted. The W (29226) is the proportion of disorganized to organized coping resources. The 22 is calculated by adding together all of the animal movetent, inanimate moverent, shading, texture, vista, and achromatic color determinants (SumEM+M+ Sum of! +I+y +c'). TheEA is calculated by adding together the sum of human moveient and weighted sum of the chromatic color determinants (Sum M plus the Sum weighted C) . The mm (21) scorings refer to responses manifesting any of the following: perseveration, a cognitive inflexability; deviant verbalization, distorted language usage or idiosyncratic modes of expression; incongruous corbination, condensation of images into an incongruous percept; fabulized corbination, implausible relationships; autistic logic, strained unconventional reasoning; or contamination, the fusion of two or more impressions of a single blot area. The total number of such responses are counted (see Appendix B for further scoring information). Win, (In) is the proportion of mature responses to total number of responses. Mature responses are those in which portions of the blot area are perceptually articulated and integrated or coibined into a single percept, or a discrete area of the blot is selected and articulated so as to erphasize the gross outline and obvious structural features of the area selected (Exner, Weiner, & Schuyler, 1978). 43 The W1 (EL) content scoring is calculated by adding the total number of units, where either one or two units are given for each hostility-evincing response, depending upon the degree of hostility expressed. Hostility is scored for (a) hostility expressed or implied, (b) hostile expressive behavior, (c) responses symbolizing hostility, and (6) objects of aggression (see Appendix C for further information on scoring). The W (AL) content scoring is scored similarly to that for hostile content. Anxiety is scored for (a) anxiety expressed or implied, (b) anxious expressive behavior, (c) responses symbolizing anxiety, and (d) cultural stereotypes of fear (see Appendix D for further scoring information. In c Interscorer reliability for the two judges, regarding the TAT and Rorschach protocols, was measured by a product-motent correlation. Scorer reliability was .82 for the pathogenesis scoring of the TAT. The following correlation coefficients reflect interscorer reliability computed from the Rorschach protocols: m, = .90, AL = .88, 20. = .80, m = .89, M = .81, R = 1.0, gpLEA = .73, and QT = .79. Judgeiental differences in the scorings were discussed and a consensus was reached for all such scores. The internal consistency for the teachers' ratings was determined by the Rude-Richardson method (Ebel, 1965, pp. 326-330). Coefficient alpha equalled .62. The average of the teachers' ratings for each daughter was used in the statistical analysis. WW To determine whether the observed correlations between pathogenesis scores, Rorschach scores, and teachers' ratings might 44 actually be attributable to their sharing a common relationship with a third factor rather than to a causal relationship between the two variables of research interest, information was obtained on variables which might affect the variables of interest. It was considered that the magnitude of a Rorschach variable might be greater in protocols with a larger number of total responses therefore 3 was correlated with other Rorschach variables (see Table 2). The variables :11 and m are positively correlated with A (r s .55 and .56, respectively, p < .05). In further corputations involving Edi and m, B was partialed out. The variables 21, my and AL were significantly related to R in the negative direction Since Rorschach protocols with fewer respmses tended to have more incidences of QT, EL, and AL (r = -.61 and -.67, p < .01; and r = -.49, p < .05, in order, the significant correlation cannot be due to productivity. The association between higher 3 and fewer AL, flu and 121‘. responses seems to be the result of the sore factor, which is better ego functioning. Likewise, the tendency for protocols with lower 3 to have higher ET, AL, and EL may be due to the clients' more disturbed ego functioning. Thus, 3 was not partialled out from further computations involving AL, HL, andQT. 45 Table 2 WW Score DI BL AL some as M m 3 -.61** -.67** -.49* -.16 .55* -.27 .56* Mote. All p's are two-tailed tests of significance. * p< .05. ** p< .01. The Thorndike Vocabulary Test (Miner, 1957) (see Appendix F) was administered to measure the daughters' intelligence. It provided an increasing score with increasing "intelligence." When correlated with pathogenesis scores, negative correlations were obtained (see Table 3) and when correlated with the teachers' ratings, a positive correlation was obtained (see Table 4); however, none of these correlations reached the .05 significance level. Socio—economic status (SIS) was another variable expected to influence the variables of interest. The Hollingshead Index of Social Position (Hollingshead & Redlich, 1958), which is based on a weighted corbination of the fathers' level of occupation and of education, produces lower Index ratings for higher social standing. Correlations between SES and the parents' _P_I scores were not significant; however the correlation between SES and fathers' 21 score (r = .34) and SEE 46 .Hoo. v .m «as .Ho. v.m as .mo. v.m « .820th my mafiaaoumS consume-8o 383m .oocmowmecwwm mo mumou poawmu-osu mum mum HH<_ .ouoz :i .8.- mm. rear *3.- «3.- «on. 8.- tamer-am. 2. «me.- tefir int-Mm u. a E E E on 2 era a some as. .E 8 sm mum moamwaoomoaa easement .mhmaumm .mumauaz_ - owmno>e xwch Home HaH _£omncmhom emu; amm~samos> -mwfidom smashes mmm .mopcom moccaeaaoucH .Hm .moanmwum> somnomuom pom mmcaumm .mumcomoH consume mcoeomaouuoo a.oaome .Ho>oH no. one on useoewecwwm uoc one mcowumaouuoo one .ouoz umoH 8.- 2.- 3.- gasses, oprEHQSH Ho Ho Ho oucom Hmuconnm .mponumm .muonyoz owmuoa< mohoom uon oocwmwaaoucu .wnounwsma tow noncom Hm Hmucwumm consume mocwumaouhoo m «one. 47 and average parental 2; score (r = .36) were close to the critical .05 level for a one-tailed test (r s .38)1 (see Table 5). Table 5 WW Average Mothers ' Fathers ' Parental Score PI PI PI Holl ing shead Index of SES . 24 . 34 . 36 me. The correlations are not significant at the .05 level. The correlation between SES and the teachers' rating also did not reach statistical significance (see Table 4). 1Since correlations between SES and both fathers' and average parental PI scores were close to, but still fell short of, the critical .05 level of significance, correlations between these pathogenesis scores and the daughters Rorschach scores were corputed, partialing out the effects of SES (see Appendix G) . Correlations between these variables, {vith the effects of SES not partialed out, may be found in Table 6, in the Results section. 48 REHJLTS The findings are sunmarized in Tables 6, 7, 8, and Figure 1. The strength of association between measures was determined by Pearson :2 whenever linearity could be assumed (McNenar, 1962). Otherwise, the correlation ratio was used (Guilford & Fruchter, 1973) . The first hypothesis, which stated that there is a positive relationship between parental pathogenesis and indices of poor ego functioning in the daughters, was confirmed. More specifically: Hypothesis 1A: ”There is a positive correlation between the pathogenesis score for the parents and (a) anxiety level score, (b) hostility level score, (c) ratio of £9135, and (d) the indices of disordered thinking for the daughters.“ The above indices of the daughters' ego functioning correlated significantly in the expected direction with the average pathogenesis score. Hypothesis 1B: ”There is a negative correlation between the pathogenesis score for the parents and m, M, m, and B for the daughters.” The m, m, and B correlated significantly in the expected direction with the average pathogenesis score. The u did 2It is usually assumed that normality and hanoscedasticity are required for proper use of 1. However, Pitman (1937) proved, by use of the randomization criterion, that r_ can be used as the most efficient nonparametric test for association, and that the resulting coefficient fran any shape distribution can be tested for significance by referral to the standard tables for r (the distribution of 1's generated by randomization closely approximates the distribution of 1's generated by normal curve theory). 49 not correlate in the expected direction and did not reach the critical .05 level of significance. Hypothesis 1C: “There is a negative correlation between the pathogenesis score for the parents' and teachers' ratings of the daughters' school performance." The teachers' ratings correlated significantly in the expected direction with the mothers', the fathers', and the average pathogenesis score. Hypothesis 1D: "The teachers' ratings of better school performance correlate positively with the Rorschach indices of better ego functioning for the daughters." The following indices of the daughters' ego functioning correlated in the expected direction: 121. EL, AL, B, m, and 131; however, 132 did not reach the critical .05 level of significance. The MBA and 1!, did not correlate in the expected direction and were not significant. The second hypothesis, which stated that the negative relationship of pathogenesis in the mothers to the daughters' ego functioning is greater than that of the fathers', was not confirmed. More specifically: Hypothesis 2A: "The pathogenesis scores for the mothers, in contrast to the fathers, correlate more highly with the indices of ego functioning in the daughters." The mothers' PI correlated more highly than fathers' PI with the following Rorschach variables: DI, epAEb, R, and m. The father's PI correlated more highly than mothers' PI with the following: EL, AL, and 2'13.- A test of difference determined that correlations involving mothers' and fathers' PI were significantly different fran one another only with respect to E11;- 50 ‘ -.- D O - -- ‘ A ' - - .1-. 0 1 ..,- .1..9A\.La’kt.. i.-.,.°_o._00.9, We: Table 4 reveals eight strong correlations in the directions that support the hypothesis that parental pathogenesis is significantly associated with poor ego functioning in the daughters. Higher teachers' ratings, which indicated better performances, were associated with lower levels of disordered thinking (121:) (r = -.65, p < .01), hostility (EL) (r = -.74, p < .001), and anxiety (AL) (r = -.48, p < .05) in the students. The proportion of the daughters' disorganized to organized coping resources score (gum) and nunber of human movement responses (L1) did not reach significance (r = .13 and -.08, respectively). The number of Rorschach responses (3) and proportion of appropriate pure form responses (23) were positively correlated with teachers' ratings (r = .58 and .59, in that order, p < .01) . The correlations between the teachers' ratings and Fit and (m) are partial correlations with the effects of 3 controlled. Controlling for the effects of R lowered the correlation with m fran (r = .72, p < .001) to (r = .59, p < .01) and lowered the correlation withm from (r = .57, p < .01) to (r = .36, n.s.). The teachers' ratings add observational data that contribute to the validity of the parental pathogenesis indices (PI) as well as Rorschach DI, EL, AL, 3, and 13.3.- Both mothers' and fathers' £1 correlated negatively with teachers' ratings of the daughters (r = -.49, in both cases, p < .05); however when the average of the mothers' and fathers' 2; scores were used in the statistical analysis the level of significance was increased (r = -.62, p < .01). 51 Table 6 smrmarizes the results of correlations between the parents' pathogenesis scores on the TAT and the Rorschach indices of the daughters' ego functioning. Partial correlation coefficients are presented for those Rorschach variables which correlated positively with R. All Rorschach variables correlate with 21 scores in the predicted direction except )1, which was expected to correlate negatively with pathogenesis scores. Table 6 W W Score 1:: EL AL 221% B m 14 m Mother .58*** .62*** .44* .51** -.59*** -.13a .40* -.57a** Father .44* .68**** .59*** .26 -.29 -.7oa**** .02 -.398* Parental Average .65*** .81**** .66*** .49** -.56*** -.60a*** .27 -.6Za*** Note. All p‘s are one—tailed tests of significance. aPartial correlation coefficients (3 controlled). Mothers' PI significantly correlates with all Rorschach variables except E13,: 121 (r = .58, p < .01), BL (r = .62, p < .01), AL (r = .44, p < .05), 312135 (r = .51, p_ < .025), R (r = -.59, p < .01), EB. (r = —.13, n.s.), n (r = .40, p < .05), and m (r = -.57, p < .025). The fathers' EL significantly correlates with five of the eight Rorschach variables: 21: (r = .44, p < .05), m (r = .68, p < .001), AL (r = 52 .59, p < .01), MBA (r = .26, n.s.), R (r = -.29, n.s.), B}. (r = -.70, p < .001), 111 (r = .02, n.s.), and m (r = -.39, p < .05). The data suggest that 39121.): R, and 14 correlate with mothers' 2; but not fathers' 21; whereas :11 correlates with fathers' EL and not with mothers' 2;. For other variables (m, an, AL, and m) the strength of the correlations differ between fathers' and mothers' 2;. Mothers' £1 correlates more strongly with 12! and m, and fathers' P; correlates higher with H]; and AL. In four instances, when the average parental EL was correlated with the Rorschach variables, higher correlations were produced than those found with either mothers' or fathers' EL Average parental EL produced significant correlations with all variables except :4: 121! (r = .65, p < .01). an (r = .81, p < .001). AL (r = .66, p < .01), some (r = .49, p < .025), B (r = -.56, p < .01), m (r = -.60, p < .01), M (r = .27, n.s.), and m (r = -.62, p < .01). The hypothesis that there is a positive correlation between parental pathogenesis and indices of poor ego functioning in the daughter was essentially confirmed. The data also show that pathogenesis in the father is a factor that contributes to poor ego development. Pathogenesis in the mother may be more significantly influential since mothers' £1, correlates significantly with seven Rorschach variables; whereas fathers' EL correlates significantly with five. The data does not confirm hypothesis 2A which states that the pathogenesis scores for the mothers, in contrast to the fathers, correlate more highly with indices of the daughters' ego functioning, since in three instances it is the fathers' pathogenesis scores which correlate more highly whereas the remaining five indices of ego functioning correlate more highly with mothers' pathogenesis score. 53 In order to determine if the correlations between the daughters' Rorschach variables and the mothers' PI were significantly different from those with the fathers' PI, the r's were transformed to their corresponding 2 values and the zobs were computed. A non-directional test at the .05 level requires an observed zobs in excess of 1 1.96. As can be seen from Table 7, only the correlations with Fit reach significance, suggesting that the probability is very remote that these two correlation coefficients could be drawn from a cannon population purely on the basis of sampling error. Table 7 121 EL AL 39135 B 2+3 2! m Z .55 '030 -060 087 101 -2015 1018 069 obs The other contradiction of the hypotheses involves the Rorschach variable M which was expected to correlate negatively with EL, with some possible variability. To help clarify the relationship between a and 21 the data were plotted (see Figure l) . Average parental 21 scores were grouped and points within the columns were averaged to produce the line of best fit. To explore the possibility of a significant nonlinear relationship, the correlation ratio was performed (n = .40, n.s.). The data were also split at the median average EL score, and a correlation coefficient was determined for each half. The lower average 2; scores correlated negatively with u (r = -.25, ms.) , and the higher average 21 scores correlated positively with M 54 .ufl umob mo 9H3 on“ mfiufimumw on meg soon 965 map—5H8 on“. EH3 350mH .ouoom mwmocwwosumm H3533 owmsog can omcommmu 38966 82!: . 33% :83qu 3595333 och. pmwmpmté mouoom mammgweumm .mumfimm paw m . .8an mmfaw. owfon. mmfam. 05:00. mofao. 83.3. mmfam. enfolu. 33.3. 33.3. mm....nm. .H mamas 83H nuanaAqq W111 JO IBQLUHN stooonoxd qoeqosxog ' Slanqfineq sq: u; sesigod 55 Table 8 Descriptive Data on the Subject Population1 Path. Score Fathers Path. Score Mbthers Average Path. Score Teacher's Rating Intelligence Adults in Home Parental GP/EA Children in Home FtZ 11 HQ 1.2 18 .76 3 .72 m w \J \D .81 3.0 .o ~4 U #40) 2 5 10 11 1.1 17 .78 4 .76 .70 .69 .70 2.3 7 69 2 sis uncle 2.2 6 92 1 bro 2.9 8 73 3.4 7 83 3.0 9 64 2.4 8 55 1 sis 2.3 8 73 J sis grand- anther 2.8 7 78 Ffinister lfiather 10 11 .90 .89 20 19 .82 .81 .95 .97 .53 .38 .43 3.2 3.5 74 32 Father Uhcle rtnixn: Father Grand- anther Mother Father Neighbor lumber Father Aunt Mother Father TeacherN Ptmher Father Neighbor Mother Father Aunt Ptmher Father Aunt htnfiex Father Uhcle Pbmher Father TeacherM Mother Father Neighbor 12 2 9 10 .97 19 .80 3 .89 .60 .33 .47 2.7 7 55 13 0 5 9 .93 20 .85 2 .85 .44 .43 .44 3.5 8 78 aunt (18 yrs} 14 2 7 8 .95 18 .83 3 .94 .47 .31 .39 3.5 6 63 1 bro 15 1 6 7 .88 20 .82 2 .90 .35 .39 .37 3.6 7 54 1 sis 16 0 6 5 .92 20 .80 2 .85 .28 .47 .37 2.9 9 69 17 0 5 8 .90 19 .83 3 .95 .33 .35 .34 3.4 10 87 18 1 6 8 .89 18 .84 3 .89 .44 .20 .32 3.2 8 73 1 bro 19 2 3 9 .93 21 .88 2 .90 .35 .29 .32 3.7 11 32 adopted 1 sis 20 0 4 6 .90 21 .86 3 .95 .31 .30 .31 3.8 8 51 Median 1.3 7.5 8.8 .93 19 .80 3 .89 .47 .44 .48 3.1 8 71 1Ordered from highest to lowest average narental pathogenesis score. 2Subjects were lZ-year-old fenales. 56 (r = .65, p < .05). That is, in the higher range of average 2;, a greater 21 score was associated with larger :4. Since higher correlations were found more often between average 21_scores, rather than either the mothers' or fathers' 21_scores, and other variables, the data was reordered from highest to lowest average 21 score to reveal any trends in the actual scores and demographic variables that.may have occurred. Table 8 lists the 20 female seventh grade subjects beginning with the subject.whose parents' average 21 score was the highest and ending with the subject.whose parents' average 21 score was the lowest. Reading across the table it may be observed that the Rorschach of the subject whose parents produced the highest average 21 score (.81) produced 4 responses reflecting disordered thinking GQT), 11 responses reflecting hostile content (HLJ, 11 responses reflecting anxious content (AL), an en greater than EA (MA = 1.2), 18 total responses (3), a proportion of adequate pure form responses (Fit = .76), 3 human movement (M) responses, and a proportion of good developmental quality location selections (DQ_= .72). Her mother and father each scored high in pathogenesis (21 = .83 and .79, respectively). The average of her teachers' ratings was 3 on a scale from 1 to 4. She answered 9 items correctly out of 20 on the Thorndike Intelligence Test. Her family socio-economic status was computed to be 73 (note that lower scores reflect higher SES). Besides herself there were three brothers and one sister at home, and no other adults except the mother and father. When asked who the three most important adults to her were, she responded: her mother, father, and a male neighbor. Thus, the table can be read across to see the actual data on each subject. Additionally the 57 columns can be scanned downward to see how the actual scores vary in relation to the decreasing average 21, scores. Wayne The Rorschach variables 121‘, EL, AL, 3213A, R, and 129, correlated, in accord with prediction, with the mothers' 2; scores; whereas EB. did not reach significance and M correlated in the opposite direction. The fathers' 2; scores correlated, as expected, with DI, EL, AL, F11, and 129.- Correlations with the following variables were not significant: £13138. 8. and 11- Using the average of the mothers' and the fathers' 21 scores in the statistica1 analysis resulted in an increase in the nunber of significant correlations, in the expected direction. Average parental 21 scores correlated with 1211, EL, AL, EPZEB: .8. £23.. and 112. When the data was split at the median, the 10 higher average parental 21 scores correlated positively with 21; whereas the 10 lower scores resulted in a nonsignificant correlation. The teachers' ratings correlated in accord with prediction, with the mothers', fathers', and average 2; scores, 12:11, EL, AL, 3, and F11. The variables MBA. 14. and 133 did not reach significance. 58 DIWSSICN The results provided support for the hypothesis that pathogenesis in the parents is associated with indices of poor ego functioning in the daughters. The justification for the above conclusion regarding the validity of the hypothesis will be apparent once the results are considered in detail, in particular, the results concerning the relatively high M, response in daughters of highly pathogenic parents, which will be considered in a later section of the discussion. The hypothesis stating that higher positive correlations would be found between maternal pathogenecity, versus paternal pathogenecity, and indices of the daughter's poor ego-functioning, were not confirmed. High correlations were found between several Rorschach factors and fathers' PI scores. The reason why some Rorschach variables correlated more highly with mothers' PI and others with fathers' PI cannot be answered by this study. It is important to note that only mothers' and fathers' PI scores correlated with Efl were found to be significantly different from one another. I can only speculate that this difference may be due to extraneous factors or individual characteristics within the particular population sampled or to the differential effects of male versus female parent due to culturally acquired roles and/or genetic factors. Information regarding other adults and children in the hane and the daughters' selection of the three most inportant adults in her life was obtained to help explain the results. Specifically, 59 nonsignificant findings might have been explained by a large proportion of the daughters relying on adults other than the mother and father in important ways. However, in accord with the significant findings, the data reveal (see Table 8) that the mother and father were routinely included in the three most important adults selected. . C ' :7 -. . .0 . 9A‘!.“. .2 ‘ . .0. .9 ‘ '10 l'et. Wings W. Tables 4, 6, and 8 show that subjects A. -_ 9!. whose parents score low in pathogenesis attain relatively low BL scores, suggesting that they harbor few feelings of resentment and enmity. In contrast, the subjects whose parents scored high in pathogenesis reveal distorted attitudes toward people. They speak of figures being at the mercy of unknown or unreal attackers, make critical or derogatory statements, and give synbolic responses sUggesting hostility. The students giving fewer of these signs of hostility were rated more favorably by their teachers, suggesting that these feelings of resentment and enmity interfere with the students' ability to perform according to their potential. W. The daughters of parents who scored low on the PI, were able to maintain a logical or realistic way of dealing with their experiences, normal for their age group. At the other extreme, the daughters of high PI parents showed some impairment to their cognitive operations. There was a tendency for these subjects to reveal a kind of cognitive inflexibility which manifested itself as a strong preoccupation with certain themes. They tended to use distorted language, condensed their impressions and ideas into percepts that violated realistic considerations, or explicated a strained reasoning when justifying an answer. These 60 signs of disordered thinking correlated negatively with the teachers' ratings, that is, students who showed little or no signs of 121 were rated more highly by their teachers. MW. Low PI parents had daughters who scored relatively low in AL, reflecting their levels of internal arousal or anxiety. These subjects also performed better in school according to the teachers' ratings. Responses suggesting danger, fear, and unpleasant emotions, either explicitly or symbolically, were found more often in the protocols of daughters whose parents scored relatively high on the PI and who were rated more poorly by their teachers. MW. According to the distribution of m scores, the daughters of low PI parents appear to be brighter and more psychologically "effective." The more primitive, rigid, diffuse sort of cognition reflecting the lower cognitive functioning expressed by low 132 was found more frequently among the daughters of those who scored relatively high on the PI. W. Low £11 reflects a disturbance in the reality testing functions of the ego and is one of the hallmarks of psychosis. This loss of accuracy in perception was encountered more frequently in the daughters of parents, particularly fathers, who scored higher on the PI. 0n the other hand, daughters of lower scoring parents, by scoring relatively high in Efl, reveal greater capacity for a critical and objective appraisal of stimuli. These subjects were more able totdirect their attention and ideation to the elements of control, discriminating judgement, and regard for the standards of the environment. It is not surprising to find that teachers tended to rate the performance of these students better. 61 We. The danghters of parents, particularly mothers, who scored relatively low on the PI, were more productive, suggesting a certain flexibility of response and tolerance of ambiguity that enabled them to utilize their perceptual-cognitive resources with efficiency. These daughters tended to be more often rated by their teachers as working closer to their potential. The B correlates positively with teachers' ratings and negatively with PI scores, although the fathers' PI does not reach the accepted level of significance. Pathogenesis in the mother may be more influential on determining the level of this factor. The average PI significantly correlates in the negative direction with 3, suggesting that relatively high PI parents have daughters who are less able to respond to the task of taking the Rorschach which required a measure of ingenuity in a novel situation, a flexibility of attitudes, tolerance of ambiguity, and an expenditure of effort (Neiger, Sleman, & Quirk, 1965) . That is, mastery and canpetence and adaptive regression in the service of the ego, the principle ego functions expressed in B, correlate positively with teachers' ratings and negatively with PI. W The em correlated positively with PI, although the fathers' PI did not reach significance; whereas this factor seemed to have little impact on the teachers' ratings. The data suggest that parents scoring lower on the PI have daughters whose affects and inner experience are more organized and available to them. In contrast the daughters of higher scoring parents have more unorganized needs and painful affects that work on the individual rather than for her. Since these canponents are relatively stable over time unless sane reconstructive impact occurs (Exner, 1974) , there is reason for concern for those subjects whose e2 62 exceeds EA. These subjects are likely to benefit from long-term psychotherapy in which they would be helped to organize more resources. W. In this study, the M response seems to represent diverse psychological activity. The unexpected 1’, results may be explained in the following manner. The daughters of parents scoring relatively low on the PI were able to engage in the "internalization" phenomenon, represented by M, in a way which was also affectively adaptive (Klopfer, 1954) . For these subjects a delay in response may be provoked by sophisticated cognitive functions that permit a more consciously determined response to occur. These M's tended to occur to whole 01) or carmon detail (Q) areas and convey a sense of cooperativeness; whereas the 24's of subjects whose parents scored relatively high on the PI, frequently occurred to unusual detail (pd) areas and involved aggressive and/or frightening content. Contrasting the :1 form quality of the four subjects whose parents produced the highest average PI with the four whose parents had the lowest average PI, we find that the proportion of good form 21 answers is .71 for the former and .82 for the latter. This difference is similiar to the differences in E3. found in these groups. The low correlation found for nunber of M responses may be due to the inclusion in the sample of both, subjects whose low number of 3 responses (see subjects 7 and 8, Table 8) and subjects whose higher nunber of :4 responses, reflect disturbed ego functioning (see subjects 1-4) . These latter subjects will be considered in more detail in a later section of this discussion. W The present study adds support to the theories of Mahler (1975), Lidz (1965, 1973), and Masterson (1976), which focus on the influence 63 of parents in determining the psychological adjustment of their offspring. A common theme which runs through all of their descriptions of faulty parent-child interactions is that when the parents' ego needs conflict with those of the child, thejparent will try to meet their own needs regardless of the effects on the child. For example, this is evident in Mahler's (1975) description of a mother who interrupts her child in his autonomous activity tO‘piCk him up and hug him because she needs it, or in Lidz's (1965) description of a father who derogates the mother, who disagrees with him, and tries to influence the daughter to take sides with him against the mother, while he also tries to use the daughter as an emotional replacement for the wife. Mahler's studies involve nonclinical samples, Masterson's are focused on outpatients diagnosed as borderline, and the offspring in Lidz's studies are schizophrenic. The common theme is the motivational pattern or attitude of the parent. By scoring for the pathogenic attitude and correlating it with various signs of the daughters' ego functioning, this study has shown that parental pathogenesis positively correlates with signs of disturbed ego functioning, and although this does not prove the above theorists to be correct, it adds support for their theories. We In essence the pathogenic parent.defends against his or her own anxieties by taking from the child; whereas the more benign parent interacts more often in the best interests of the child. Pathogenesis is conceived of as a set of defenses which the parent is not generally aware of. By using others in their defense against their own problems, 64 pathogenic parents may be able to ward off more overt signs of pathology in themselves. The present study confirms that a range of pathogenesis scores may be found within a nonclinical population. However, it cannot be said that the daughters, of the parents who scored high in pathogenesis, have pathology, since none of them were grossly impaired, had been hospitalized for a psychological disturbance and each one had attained the expected grade level for her age. Outwardly, each one seemed to be performing adequately. A longitudinal study would indicate whether the daughters of high pathogenesis parents were more likely to develop serious psychopathology in the future. The teachers' ratings were not specifically a measure of pathology. These behavioral observations more accurately assessed the students' ability to be productive, adapt to the situation, perceive accurately, and organize and canmunicate ideas effectively - all of which are aspects of the individuals' ego functioning and are reflected in several of the Rorschach variables. One of the Rorschach variables reflects a type of primitive drive-daninated thinking (Elizur's Hostility Scale), which sane authors have related to desireable personality characteristics. For example, Woody (1981) notes Getzels and Jackson's study where among subjects of above average intelligence, highly creative (and relatively less intelligent) adolescents expressed more aggression and violence than less creative (and more intelligent) adolescents. However, Woody also mentions several other studies in which primitive drive-daninated thinking was not associated with creativity and Gray's suggestion that the positive relationship found between creativity and primary process is the result of a canmon variable, productivity. Woody also notes a 65 study by Rychlack and O'Leary in which more socially inappropriate drive material came fran lower-SE5 children than higher—SE8 children, and was related “to the fact that low-SES people are Open, honest, sincere, and straightforward." In response to the suggestion that high scores on Elizur's Hostility Scale are a sign of creativity, honesty, and sincerity, observe that Elizur's hostility content scoring has been modified to emphasize particular kinds of hostile content associated with psychopathology in children (Crain & Smoke, 1981) and does not measure aggressive drive, per se. The present study considers relatively high hostility level (11,) scores a sign of poor ego functioning, particularly when accanpanied by poor form and developmental quality, elevated g2 in relation to EA, and indices of disordered thinking because these latter signs indicate that the individual's higher cognitive functioning is unable to exert adequate control over the primitive drive-related mentation. The present study, being correlational, does not confirm that pathogenic parents cause the disturbances in ego functioning that their daughters seem to have. The possible contribution of genetic factors has not been ruled out. Moreover there may be sane influence fran the daughter which "pulls," to sane degree, the pathogenic style of relating from the parents. Nonetheless, the most probable interpretation of the data is that the defenses of the parents cause, at least to sane degree, the ego disturbance of the daughters. It is hypothesized that the lZ-year-old daughter is no longer simply an innocent victim of deprivation and is very much involved in the quest to satisfy her security needs, bolster her faltering ego and defend against further onslaught. Her perceptions of the world are 66 defined by the first world she knew, that which existed within her earliest experience in the family, and her means of dealing with that world were learned during her development by observing the defenses used by her parents. If she can survive the pathogenic family environment by not becaning psychotic, by separating enough to establish a family of her am, she may be freer to use the defenses she has learned from her parents and becane the pathogenic parent of the next generation. Previous research (Meyer & Karon, 1967; Mitchell, 1968, 1969) has emphasized the significant connection between the pathogenic mother and schizophrenia in the offspring. While the present study is in accord with those studies, it highlights the importance of the father for adequate ego functioning in adolescent daughters. Theoretically, the essential role of the father in the development of the child has been focused on in Lidz's (1965) study of seventeen families with schi20phrenic offsprings. The seventeen fathers were found to exert important and often disturbing influence within the family. Lidz (1965) contends that the father plays an essential role in the development of the child. He is the first intruder into the child's feelings of unity with the mother, and the child should identify and develop a feeling of security with him. The father also has an inportant influence on the mother's ability to be secure in her mothering role. Fathers who resented and became rivals of the child or of the mother were cannon in Lidz's study. These fathers were often unable to support the mother in her role and share her with the child. The mother is the main figure for identification for the daughter, however the worth of the mother as a love object and a sheltering 67 figure cannot be separated from the father's attitude toward her. In order for her to be an adequate model for identification for the daughter, the mother must be appreciated by the father. The father should also be a suitable early love object, so that the daughter may seek to gain the love of a man by becaning a woman like the mother; but at the same time there must be no confusion of the generations between parents and children. For normal development to occur, the child must know that s/he cannot take one parent's place with the other. Fathers studied by Lidz (1965) whocanpeted as a son of the wife, or remained a passive child displaced in the wife's affection by his child, or, at the other extreme, sought to substitute a daughter or son for his wife, threatened the division between generations which offered the child security against incestuous and castration anxieties. For example, the fathers of sane of the female schizophrenic patients were in constant conflict with their wives, undercutting their authority and derogating their human worth, while at the same time favoring the daughter and trying to win her over to their own side of the controversy. Furthermore, the fathers were highly inconsistent and their demands were dependent upon their manentary neurotic or psychotic needs. Lidz (1965) concludes, based on a survey of fifty families, that ". . .the paternal influences are noxious as frequently as are the maternal.” In accord with his conclusions, the present study has revealed several strong correlations between father's, as well as mother's, PI scores and indices of the daughter's ego functioning . 68 ' n' ' F P oco s Since the result involving the M score was not as predicted, the individual Rorschach protocols were examined more closely to shed light on the meaning of the M result. The four protocols belonging to subjects 1, 2, 3, and 4 (see Table 8) had relatively high L1 associated with high parental pathogenesis. A closer look reveals that on these protocols color is rarely integrated into the responses, animal movement (EM) is relatively low and inanimate movement (m) is elevated, as well as achranatic color 51, shading (X), and texture (I) . The excessive Q is thought to reflect "feelings of rejection often expressive of true depression" (Francis-Williams, 1968). The excessive 1 (i.e., smoke, clouds) may be an expression of uncertainty and anxiety which is supported by AL. The low EM may suggest a paucity of spontaneity and zest, unusual for a person of this age. Color, when it occurs, is often not incorporated into a scorable response (i.e., "I like pink"). One subject produced no color responses, which suggests a failure in normal growth in flexibility and adaptability and perhaps a retreat fran relations with the outside world and a repression or denial of the auctions normally aroused by color. For these four subjects M may reflect the ability to use fantasy as a defense mechanism: an attempt to cope with the pressures of the environment by apparent conformity while protecting an inner system of values or by presenting sanething of their own inner life and withdrawing from the reality of the test situation. Several of these )1 responses involved poor use of form, human details (i.e., a fist with a nail protruding fran between the fingers), and sinister or unrealistic figures (i.e., "the shadow lord"). There was also a tendency to relate the 11 response to small or unusual 69 details and to perseverate on certain themes (i.e., two people connected together as a single entity and ”smashed“ guitars, butterflies, bears, etc.). The increased use of blackness and reduction in color suggests a tendency to perceive the‘world as an unsafe place to allow much feeling--it suggests the character of a limited and emotionally impoverished person. One of these subjects, who'was apparently intelligent, seemed to struggle to retain a semblance of normality throughout the test. She gave on the whole, a mixture of very good and very poor form level. Her response to'Card‘v, which follows, suggests that the emotions aroused by the cards had for the moment.broken down her capacity for good intellectual functioning. "It looks like it has something to»do with a rabbit. There's the ears and there's the legs and here's flight. It's in flight. It's going. It's fright too because the ears are straight up. I can see the back of his head focused ahead and all this in here is unidentifiable flesh. He must have rabbit's feet-paws. A.couple of different sets of them." In the inquiry, she continued, ”I can see fear in here. He's alert, his ears are alert, and his feet, they look scared. Here (she points to the trunk of the body) this looks dead. You see a rabbit's leg and some flesh and blood or something, some part of his body. I can't.identify how it all fits together, but it's rabbitlike. This is the back of him and he's in motion, this part (points to the head), but this part (points to the trunk of the body) is not moving, its lying on the ground.” Her response to this card, most popularly responded to as anbat or a butterfly, reflects soner"cognitive slippage." She has failed to maintain a logical or realistic way of dealing with her experience and one can speculate that it reflects a self reference. Her image of a 70 scared and wounded rabbit (ears alert, head racing in flight, legs innubilized with fear, body mutilated and dead) may reflect her reaction to a frightening environment where she must be always alert. The deadness may refer to her deadened emotions or the psychic damage that she perceives has been done to her. I would speculate that her anxiety seriously interferes with her ability to maintain adequate ego functioning and defenses under stress. As a final consideration, before leaving these subjects whose parents scored highest in pathogenesis, the first three subject's responses to the “father" and "mother” cards are considered below. Cards IV and VII "pull" for responses that.directly or symbolically relate to masculinity and femininity, respectively. The more dysfunctional a child or adolescent's relationship is with his/her parents, the more likely the child/adolescent selects Card IV as the “father" or Card‘VII as the "mother" card (Hayden, 1981). It is purported that the percepts projected onto either of these two cards represent a significant motivational process (Schafer, 1954). Subject (1) gave the following response to Card IV. "I see a giant. There's his feet and arm.and head. His head up there is really small because its far away. He's sitting on top of a tree stump that has some kind of claws. He looks like he's.been'burned. He's all crusty like charcoal. He's got a head like an insect. He's holding up two things on his shoulders. Candles, maybe.” During the inquiry, she said, "He's holding his shoulders up to support those things. He looks like a burnt piece of paper at the edges. His feet are all burnt-here to his waist." She gave the following response to Card VII. "It looks like Mt. Rushmore. There's the faces carved. YOu can only see the front part. 71 Here's a valley and water. The faces look really old. They've started to decay." During the inquiry she said, "Its darker in sane places so it looks like different faces. Maybe there's the mouth part, nose, but I can't tell, its starting to decay. All this here is the mountain. You can see down in the valley. I think its mountains because of the color, grey, like rock." Subject (2) responded in the following manner to Card IV. "Looks like the shadow lord or Darth Vader. Looks sort of evil, like a phantan. Looks like it might be a spirit. The arms are missing." She said the following during the inquiry. "The shadow lord is the ghost of the future. He doesn't have a definite shape. This is the head. This could be the eyes and the arms. It looks like just a cloak and arms, but you can't see than. This is sort of like the eyes here, but its like a mask. It seans evil because there is sanething caning out here of the arms." Her first response to Card VII was only that, "It looks like a cactus." During the inquiry she elaborated a little. "You know. It's in a pot. It has arms that grow all over the place, with prickers on them. It's bumpy all over." Subject (3) gave the following response to Card IV. "This raninds me of riding in an airplane, looking down at a river in the center with cliffs or rocks on either side because of how its darker here." During the inquiry she explained further. "Looking down you see different shades. This part looks dark to me. Just looks deeper like a river. The lighter and darker raninds me of looking down at a river and rocks and a valley." She responded in the following way to Card VII. "Well, I kind of see faces, now. Men's faces and those are ladies' faces above than, just kind of growing out of the men's faces." During the inquiry 72 she continued. "This looks like hair sticking out kind of in a pony tail. Here are the noses. These ladies heads are caning out of the men's heads. Here's hair sticking out in front, the forehead of the man's head, his eyes and nose. It's like the ladies' heads are on t0p of the men's heads-just balancing there.” Note the use of achranatic color, texture, and vista in these responses. This suggests a "toned down" affective experience and a hesitancy to directly express affect. There are needs for affective interpersonal contact, but perhaps at an immature level. Probably most salient, is the painful quality of the affective experience. The two responses including vista (Subject 1, Card VII and Subject 3, Card IV) are rare and are cause for concern. These responses suggest that the subjects are distancing thanselves from their problans. There may be a painful introspective process that involves depression of affect and feelings of inferiority. The description of the male figures (Subject 1 and 2, Card IV) suggest sinister qualities but also an apparent lack of power. For example, the giant, with the head of an insect, sitting on a stump with claws must hold his shoulders up to support candles. He's already burnt fran the waist down, and the shadow lord or Darth Vader is armless. Moreover, the substance caning out of the arms of the shadow lord, the decaying faces (Subject 1, Card VII), and the ladies' heads growing out of the men's heads (Subject 3, Card VII) suggest difficulty in the area of body boundaries, the subject's feeling that her body exterior is of little protective value (Goldfried, Stricker, & Weiner, 1971) . 73 SUM'SARY AND CMUSIGQS The present study investigated ego functioning in 12—year-old daughters whose parents revealed various degrees of pathogenesis. The nature of the problan upon which the study was focused was whether relatively high levels of parental pathogenesis were associated with poorer ego functioning in the daughters and lower levels of parental pathogenesis were associated with better ego functioning in the daughters. The question of whether pathogenesis in the mother, in contrast to pathogenesis in the father, was more closely related to the daughters' disturbance in ego functioning was also investigated. Projective tests were used since poor ego functioning might be concealed by a "false self" and pathogenesis is often accanpanied by verbalized concern about the well-being of the depaident individual (VandenBos & Karon, 1971) . Level of pathogenesis was determined by the pathogenesis index (PI) of the TAT; whereas aspects of the daughters' ego functioning were determined by the following Rorschach variables: disordered thinking, anxiety level, hostility level, number of Rorschach responses, proportion of appropriate pure form, nunber of human movanent responses, proportion of disorganized to organized coping resources, and developmental quality. Guntrip (1969) contends that poor ego development is basic and causal for all kinds of personality disorder. Mahler's (1975) studies have shown that ego development does not proceed normally when mothers interact with their young children according to their own needs to 74 maintain their equilibrium and disregard their children's needs. Moreover, Lidz (1965) points to the positive molding forces that exist within the family. The child learns to use paralogical ways of thinking, to relinquish reality-testing, to deny or distort, and to distrust verbal camnunication within the family through interactions with parents who use such defenses. It's not surprising that Albee (1969) found poorer intellectual performance in children who would later becane schizophrenic. The PI has been used successfully to differentiate groups of mothers of schizophrenics from mothers of normals (Meyer & Karon, 1967; Mitchell, 1968) and to differentiate child-abusing mothers from controls (Melnick & Hurley, 1969) . The present study extends the research on pathogenesis by investigating the range of parental pathogenesis within a nonclinical sample, and correlating the mothers', the fathers', and the average parental PI scores with the daughters' Rorschach scores. Thus, the present study looks for relatively subtle signs of the quality of ego functioning (Rorschach scores in a group of "normals” versus clinical/control groups) and relates the various levels of ego functioning to the various levels of mothers', fathers', and average PI. The fathers' pathogenesis was found to be more highly related to the daughters' ego functioning than expected. High correlations were found between fathers' PI and DI, EL, AL, £31, and m. In accord with prediction the mothers' PI correlated significantly with 121, EL AL, 22138: B. and m. The average of both parents PI led to an increase in significant correlations in the predicted direction. Average PI correlated significantly with the following Rorschach variables: 121, EL, AL, eplEA, B, 1:31, and m. Teachers' ratings of the daughters 75 corroborated the above findings since ratings correlated significantly in the expected direction with mothers' PI, fathers' PI, average parental PI, and the following Rorschach variables from the daughters' protocols: III, E, AL, 3, and L1. The M response was considered in more detail by interpreting its neaning on several individual protocols. ‘When studied in this manner, the poorer quality M responses were found among the daughters of parents who scored relatively high on the PI and the more integrated M responses were found among the daughters of parents who scored relatively low on the PI. Protocols of the four subjects whose parents scored highest on the PI were»described in more detail. Included in the description of these protocols were the subjects' responses to the mother (Card‘VII) and the father (Card IV) cards. It was concluded that there is a positive correlation between parental pathogenesis and indices of poor ego functioning in the daughters. The‘daughters' poor ego functioning was indicated by both the Rorschach scores and teachers' ratings. The study indicated that pathogenesis in the father is a factor significantly associated with poor ego functioning in the daughters. The PI and the Rorschach factors 21, AL, EL, 3, F13, and DR sean to be sensitive enough to pick up signs of pathogenesis and ego functioning, respectively, in a noncl inical population . 76 APPENDICES APPBQDIX A Projection The term projection is often considered a misnaner when applied to what has becane known as projective techniques. However, Rabin (1960) contends that critics of the use of the term "seem to be attached to a rather circumscribed definition of projection. They consider it only in a pathogenic context, i.e., as a defense mechanism described by Freud and by psychoanalytic theory. Closer examination and more canplete scrutiny of Freud's writings will reveal that he attributed a broader and more extended meaning to the term projection" (Abt & Bellak, 1950) . Rapaport (1952) has also pointed out that a whole range of phenanena is reflected in the psychoanalytic use of the term projection. He states that "...phenanena subsumed under the psychoanalytic conception of projection... (are) paranoid projection, infantile projection, projection in transference phenanena..." He further elaborates by pointing out that these phenanena "...are rather a graduated continuum which becanes progressively more general extending from the externalization of a specific type of tension in paranoid projections to that of any kind of tension in infantile projection, to that of a whole system of attitudes and tensions in transference phenanena, to where it imperceptibly shades into the externalization in the form of a 'private world' defined by the organized principles of one's personality." Rabin (1960) suggests that the broader term “externalization" is more appropriate in the case of projective techniques, since it avoids the constricting misconception of projection as a mere defense 77 mechanism. In this connection, Rabin (1960) also notes others who have been concerned with a more precise designation of the processes involved in projection techniques. Bellak (Abt & Bellak, 1950), for instance, writes of "apperceptive distortions” and Cattell (Anderson & Anderson, 1951) of "dynaception" and "misperceptions." Rabin (1960) asserts that "the need for this modified terminology is obviated when we consider the process of externalization as a continuum and its involvement in the broad range of perceptual tasks, from.the more strictly cognitive, with highly structured material, to the more apperceptive or misperceptive, with unstructured or less structured stimulus materials." 78 APPEDDIXB Exner's Scorings for Disordered Thinking Hermann: Perseveration reflects a kind of cognitive "inflexibility" which may relate to a strong preoccupation or to a more severe form of impairment in which the cognitive operation becanes very concrete. There are two kinds of perseveration either of which should be scored PSV. W: These are responses that use exactly the same location, determinant(s) , content score, DQ and FQ scores, and the sane Z and P scoring. The content may change slightly but ranains in the sane general content category. For example, ”This could be a bat,” would be scored Wo Fo A P 1.0. Following this, “It could be a butterfly too," would be scored Wo Fo A P 1.0 PSV. Won: Content perseveration may be scored for responses that are not consecutive, and may even be separated by several cards. These are answers in which the subject identifies an object as being the sane one seen earlier. The scoring may be different for the new answer than for the answer in which the object was perceived originally. For instance, a subject reports ”No people fighting" on Card II, and then, when shown Card III says, "Oh, it's the sane people, the fight is over now and they're bowing to each other." PSV would be assigned as a special score for the answer to Card III. II JlZlJ' I' There are three categories of unusual verbalizations, each of which reflects sane “strange" or disordered thinking operation. The three categories are deviant verbalizations, inappropriate cmbinations, and autistic logic. Multiple scorings should be avoided in this area, that is, the scorer should select the category which best identifies the answer for any single response. WW5: The UV answers are characterized by distorted language usage or idiosyncratic modes of expression that impede the subject's ability to cannunicate clearly. A queer response appears to result fran the subject's failure to maintain an appropriate set in talking about what he is responding to. For instance, "A crab but I was hoping for an octopus," or ”That's like a monster that no one has ever seen." The second form of deviant verbalization is the use of odd language that cannot be justified in terms of subcultural idians or limited vocabulary skills. Sane of these responses involve language that is stilted or redundant, as in, "A matched brace of stanachs with food gullets attached,“ or ”An x-ray of sanebody's self." More canmonly this form of deviant verbalization is manifest by the use of neologisms or incorrect words used in place of a correct word that seans well 79 within the subject's intellectual capacity. For exanple, ”Something in a biography lab,” or "Two dogs with their snoods together.” WW: These responses involve the inappropriate condensation of impressions and ideas into percepts that violate realistic considerations. Three separate scorings are provided to differentiate the kind of inappropriate carbination that has occurred. The W consists of the condensation of blot details or images into a single, incongruous percept. For exanple, "A person with the head of a bat," or "A four legged chicken." Sanetimes, the incongruity will be manifest by the inappropriate carbination of color with form, such as, "Blue people.“ The mom will always involve only a single object or percept. The W always involves two or more discrete blot details. FABCCN is scored when an implausible relationship is posited between two or more blot details, such as, "TWO chickens holding basketballs." The objects have been perceived in a presumed relationship to each other that cannot occur, or which is highly unlikely to occur in nature. For exanple, "TwO rats eating a wanan's ovaries," or "A snake attacking a submarine." The W is scored when two or more impressions of a single blot area are fused into a single percept that clearly violates reality and "spoils“ the adequacy of either of the impressions as contrasted with the situation where they would be reported alone. Whereas the IICCN answers fuse impressions of discrete blot areas into a single implausible percept, CONTAM responses involve the use of just one blot area to arrive at one percept, but a percept in which impressions that should be kept distinct fran each other becane condensed into a response of bizarre quality. A classic illustration of the neologistic CON'IAM is the condensation of impressions of the whole Card IV as the front of a bug and the front of an ox into, "The front of a bug—ox." A peculiarly verbalized C(NIAM to the sane card is exanplified by a subject who apparently perceived it both an an organ of the body, and as an authoritarian male figure, and responded, ”The liver of a respectable statesman." Wig: The ALCI; scoring is used whenever the subject, without pranpting, explicates a strained reasoning when justifying his anaver. The logic involved reflects a form of loose or circumstantial thinking. In most Aha; responses, the reasoning becanes attached to, and dependent upon the size, positioning, or nunber of objects included in the response. For exanple, ”This must be lettuce because it's next to the rabbits aid they eat lettuce," or "It must be a man and a wanan because they're together, and men and wanen belong together,“ or "It's the north pole because it's at the top." The total nunber of special scores will be tabulated for each protocol and used in the statistical analysis. 80 This scoring criteria appeared in Exner, J.E., Weiner, I.B., and SChUYler, W- (1976)- W w. New York: Rorschach Workshops. 81 APPENDIX C W The author has altered Elizur's method by scoring hostile expressive behavior h instead of H when it contains concrete interactions between equals and the outcane is partly under the figure's control. This change is based upon the findings of Crain and Smoke, 1981. Responses are scored hostility-evincing if they contain the following characteristics. mm: A capital H would be scored if hatred, dislike, criticism, or derogation were expressed in the response. Examples of these responses include "A type of man I hate"; ”an ugly figure"; ”a stupid animal"; "an angry face"; "a quarrelsane person." A lowercase 11 would be scored for responses indicating such hostility to a lesser degree. Exanples of such responses include: "gossiping wanen"; "frowning people.” WW: If the behavior of the percept figure suggests hostility, with the percepts including victims of violence without saying who the aggressor is, H would be scored. Exanples of these responses include: “He's dead, sanebody killed him"; ”they squashed the butterfly"; "a killed animal." The H would also be scored if the subject specifies the aggressor as an overpowering monster or creature that might eat one up. Exanples include: "A wolf devouring its prey"; "a big monster caning at you.” A lowercase 11 would be scored if the aggression is tied to concrete interactions in which the outcane seans at least partly under the figure's control, such as fighting between two equals. Exanples: "Two boys fighting with each other" ; "two bears fighting." W: A lowercase 11 would be scored for synbolic responses suggesting hostility. Examples: "The red represents struggle"; "a primative war mask." W: The presence of objects used for aggressive purposes would be scored H, or h. Exanples of H include: "Arrows"; ”gun"; ”pistol." Exanples of h are: "Pliers”; "knife“; "teeth." WWW: Responses that contain clear evidence of both anxiety and hostility would be scored 11. Bramples of such responses include: "An animal going to attack you, I feel sanewhat scared”; ”a policanan." Elizur believed that borderline responses in which there was doubt as to whether hostility was involved should not be scored. An individual's total hostility score is determined by counting each 11 response as two units, and each 11 response as one unit. The two resulting sums are added to yield the total hostility score. This scoring criteria appeared in Aronow, E., & Resnikoff,M . (1976), Won New York: Grune and Stratton- 82 APPENDIX D EJ° u , E . I E J S . Elizur scored responses as anxiety-evincing if they were characterized by the following features. W A capital A would be scored if fear, unpleasantness, sorrow, pity, or similar emotions were expressed in responses. Examples: "A.frightening giant“; "A.weeping child"; TA dangerous crevice"; "Darkness and gloom." A lower case a would be scored for responses indicating a lesser degree of anxiety, for example, "an unpleasant animal." W If the behavior of the percept suggested anxiety. A,would be scored. Examples of such responses include “a.girl escaping"; “a retreating animal': "a rabbit running away." 1gnnnIgnnsymbglizing_anxiaty, A lower case a,would be scored for symbolic responses suggesting anxiety, for example, "an unbalanced figure"; "dead leaves"; “scarecrow"; "an impression of coldness.” . The presence of percepts having a general connotation of fear in our culture would be scored A or a. Examples of,A include "bats," "snakes," "monsters," "atcnuc bomb. ” Examples of a are "spiders," “mosquitoes,” "church," "totem pole." Wm- Responses containing clear evidence of both anxiety and hostility were scored a, Examples of such responses include "headless person," “injured bear," "child with cut—off arms," "a torn butterfly," "a.policeman," "an animal going to attack you, I feel somewhat scared.“ In Elizur's system borderline responses in which there is doubt as to*whether anxiety is involved should not be scored. Examples presented by Elizur that suggest neither anxiety nor hostility are the following. Animals. Typical exanples are frogs, mice, bugs, crabs, and bears. Anatgmig. Spinal cords, x-ray, bones, and lungs are included here. W. Cannon responses include coats of arms, rocks, and skins of animals. A subject's total anxiety score is carputed by counting each A response as two units and each a response as one unit. The two resulting sums are added to yield the total anxiety score. This scoring criteria appeared in Aronow, E., & Resnikoff, M. (1976). W New York: Grune and Stratton. 83 APPENDIX E Scoring Criteria for the Pathogenesis Index of the TAT W 1. Murder. 2. Boss driving workers hard. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. Parents make boy study or practice when he doesn't want to. Mother supposedly kind, but not meeting expressed needs of child. Mother showing particularity for one daughter or son over another. Any kind of talking to as a form of punishment. Mother warning child on things that can harm him in growing up. Mother telling child he hasn't worked up to ability. Going to canetery to scare people. Husband gives wife news he is leaving town (or her). Spying on girlfriend, or being stood-up. Monster ready to attack child or smaller animal. Happy old witch and pretty young wanan. Man telling wife sanething to hurt her, e.g., took secretary to dinner. Mother reading to child fran Bible to teach her a lesson. Wanan and evil conscience; devil behind her; etc. Mother feels what she has said to daughter has done little good. Husband interrupts sanething wife is interested in. Nasty remarks to a subordinate, making him or her unhappy. King or leader leading nation to ruin. Mother doesn't like sanething about daughter or son (looks, make-up, attitudes, etc.) even though daughter or son likes it. Refusal of marriage bid; one is interested, one is not. Mother checking up on son or daughter (study, etc.) Destructive witch thanes. Fanily ruled or dominated. Husband or father jealous or forbidding. Woman harming child by punishment. Suicide attanpt to frighten saneone. Man pulling out of extra-marital affair and wanan doesn't want to. Losing interest in playing the violin. (It is assumed that Card 1 reveals parental relations even if the parent is not mentioned. The child playing the violin against his will is assumed to imply coercion.) W l. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. No interaction between two people, though sanewhat conflicting needs. One person enjoying himself. No people or living things. “70 people, but no indication of interaction. Conflict with person's own needs, not other people's. Thinking about a mother who was kind to her. (It is assumed that this indicates such a fantasy—but it is at least as likely to be defensive as it is to be a reflection of reality.) Wanting to join a dead person—an intrapsychic problan. 84 Benigalhemas 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. Parents force child to do sanething, he is unhappy, they change. Teacher consoling a problan child; helping a gifted child. Guides leading animals across difficult area, etc. Reunion of two people—both people pleased. Person springing a pleasant surprise on another one. Parent interrupts punishment of child by another parent. Stopping children from activity in which they would be likely to get hurt. Woman trying to console man in trouble. Father and daughter consoling each other after death of mother. Helping people at a disaster. Son or daughter interested in advice fran parents (or stories). Wanan working hard for benefit of her children. Mother thinking about children and is happy. Accepted presentation of love, or evidence of mutual love. Mother admiring work of children or making sanething they like. Man heeds wanan's wish not to leave. Any attarpt to help or console with no ulterior motive. Prevention of disaster (suicide, murder, etc.). Mother enlightening child about the birds and the bees. This scoring criteria appeared in Meyer, R.G., & Karon, B.P. (1967). The schizophrenic mother concept and the TAT. Many, 39,, 173-179. 85 APPENDIXF q wfiuaahoo .m 39.68% .c 395m .m £33ch .N @3ch .H 538mm H mmoazoowmnfla .m fiwdfiwcwfifl .d Duane—.5 .m 89% .N fiwfiflnwfis 2" E? m 53w .n @860. .q ant/mafia .m “Simon .N gamma A mag m 88398 .m coammwpm .q catamaran .m vacuum .N concunuoamw .H E m pocguom .m bonds: .1» 859.8 .m 850.85 .N 333:? .H EHQU a a ucmsaookm . m 33.? .q cannon: . m E385: .N wcom .H 53% m 850 . m Hoe/nu . a ugga . m moum .N mood—Eco . H 5% H monocfiwum .m banana/Hp .q 85330ch .m cowumficn .N coupes .H E; a #88365 .m 5.51; .1» oomfiww .m noun n .N «Bag .H mug m 88.8me .m 88.8 .q 1338 .m 5833 .N mean A 20334.2 0 H manmucoo .n comma: .e cannons .m 588.20 .m 893 A ”533.com m . oncmmmua .m amigo .c Daemouwn .m oQfiwumCQBH .N mocnmmea .H Foam m xenon on «Jun .m gunman .q com on new .m oafimfio .N mcowoeamcn .H 555 s wanen .m eds .e manna .m diam .N 6%... A Baas a mmofimcn. .m poms pow IV Hougonm .m gamed .N “.509“me .H Educ m H be: .m 358 .e n88 .m seas .N :8 4 Spam m 8e? .m 88886 .q 883 .m Eon mama .N 8&8 4 2.88% m fins 8 8 9m; .m pecans .e 5363 .m mammal .N 3.336 was A zmmozoo N .Aocnm .m >958 .c o3n> .m omemu .N “50 upon .H EA a whoop .m soon .1» 5398 .m Coos .N Hoonom .H moamm < mg uoouuoo noon. Engage, oprEofi. urn. 2 was 86 APPENDIX G Score DE EL AL M B Efl H m Father .46b* .63b** .59b** .26 -.29 -.66ab** .02 -.33ab Parental Average .69b*** .78b*** .67b** .48b* -.52b** -.54ab** .27 -.583b** Nate. All p's are one-tailed tests of significance. aPartial correlation coefficients (3 controlled). bPartial correlation coefficients (SES controlled). *2 < .025. **p_ < .01. ***n < .001. 87 APPENDH H Wings The teachers' ratings were determined fran the MzDonald Middle School Surnnary Report Form for each child. The summary report is a progress report which indicates an acadanic grade for each of the child's subjects. In a given subject or unit of study sane students may be working above grade level while others may be at or below grade level. Teachers set different objectives each reporting period for different students or groups of students, depending on what a student or group of students needs to learn next. Thus, students work at their own pace according to their abilities. Grades are a reflection of the student's progress in meeting objectives established for her. They are not measures of performance against a standard set for all children at that grade level. The following is a list of the possible grades and their meanings. A - Has met all of her objectives B - Has met all but few of her objectives C - Has met a satisfactory portion of her objectives D - Has met less than a satisfactory portion of her objectives but sane worthwhile progress did occur E - Has met very few or none of her objectives The grades were received fran seven teachers for the student's progress in the following subjects or units of study: mathanatics, science, english, social studies, physical education, plus two electives fran the following: choral, industrial arts, hananaking, or library aide. Accarpanying the grades was a narrative report, approximately four pages in length, which listed cannents related to the student's acadanic progress and statanents which indicated sane learning skills and habits and some personal and social behaviors that each teacher believed influenced the student's progress toward meeting her own objectives. For the statistical analysis, A, B, C, D, B were converted to 4, 3, 2, l, 0, respectively, and an average of the seven teachers' progress grades, was used as the teachers' rating for each student. 88 REFERENCES REFERENCES Abelin, E.L. (1971). The role of the father in the separation- individuation process. In J.B. mDevitt & C. F. Settlage (31s.), ‘01-. o. m- H 0 s '01' \ I H) 0 0 31-. 0 ‘ 4- .0 ‘ (pp. 229—253). New York: International Universities Press. Abrans, E.N. (1955). Prediction of intelligence fran certain Rorschach factors. immaLnfslinicallsyszhologx 1L 81-83. Abt, L., & Bellak, L. (1950). W. Alfred A. Knoff. WW 25112119199); by L~E- Abt- Alanen, Y. (1960). Sane thoughts on schizophrenia and ego development in the light of fanily investigations. MW Wu. 3., 650-656. Albee, G., Lane, E., Corcoran, C., 8 Werneke, A. (1963). Childhood and intercurrent intellectual performance of adult schizophrenics. MW 21. 4. 364-366. Albee, G., Lane, 15., 8 Reuter, J. (1964). Childhood intelligence of future schizophrenics and neighborhood peers. W W: .53.. 141-144- Altus, W. D. (1958). Group Rorschach and Q—L discrepancies on the ACE W A. 469. Ames, L.B., Learned, J., Metraux, R., 5. Walker, R.N. (1952). guild Wales. New York: Hoeber-Harper- Pmes, L.B. (1966). Changes in Rorschach responses throughout the human life span. Genet-W. 1.4. 89-125. Ames, L.B., Matraux, R., & Walker, R. (1971). W new. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Anderson. 6.3- (1972). WWW. Unpublished masters thesis, North Dakota State University. Anthony, E. J. (1971). Folie a Deux: A developmental failure in the process of separation-individuation. In J .B. McDevitt & C.F. Settlage (EBs.), - W (pp. 253-273). New York: International Universities Press. Arieti. S- (1955). MW. NewYork: Brunner. Aron, L. (1982) . Stressful life events and Rorschach content. W. .46. 6. 89 Aronow, E00 & Reznikoff. M. (1976). W New York: Grune and Stratton. Beck, S.J. (1945). ' ° pictures. New York: Grune and Stratton. BeCk, S. J., Beck, A.G., Levitt, E. E., & Molish, H. B. (1961). Was (3rd ed ) New York: Grune and Stratton. Beck, S.J. (1965). ' ° W. New York: Grune and Stratton. Beck S J.. & Molish. H B. (1967) BMW W (2nd ed. ). New York: Grune and Stratton. Becker, W. C. (1956). A genetic approach to the interpretation and evaluation of the process—reactive distinction in schizophrenia. MW 51. 229-236. Crain, W. C., 8 Smoke, L. (1981). Rorschach aggressive content in normal and problematic children. W Assessment 3.5. l- Cronbach, L.J. (1949). Statistical methods applied to Rorschach scores: A review. MW. 3i. 393-429. Cmnmings,C.P. (1954). 'ous col 'c v ables WW- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA. Deutsch, H. (1944). W. Vol. 1. New York: Grune and Stratton. E’oel. EL (1965). W Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Elizur, A. (1949). Content analysis of the Rorschach with regard to anxiety and hostility W 12. 247-284. Ehmer. J.B. (1974). Wen (Vol. I). New York: Wiley. Exner, J. E., Weiner, I.B., & Schuyler, W. (1978). LBQIfithQh W New York: Rorschach Work shops. Exner, J. E., & Weiner, I. B. (1978). Rorschach indices of disordered thinking in patient and nonpatient adolescents and adults. MW .42, 4. Fiske, D.W., & Baughman, E.E. (1953). Relationships between Rorschach scoring categories and the total ntmwber of responses. W W. AB. 25—32. 90 Forsyth, R.P. (1959). The influence of color, shading and Welsh anxiety level on Elizur Rorschach content test analysis of anxiety and hostility WW5. 23. 207-213. Friedman. H. (1952). W W. W W 51. 63-98. Framr-Reichmarm, F. (1960). ' ' (p. 265). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Glatt. CUP. (1971). W W Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. Glatt, C. T., & Karon, B. P. (1974) . A Rorschach validation study of the ego regression theory of psychopathology. W W .42. 569-576. Goldberger, L. (1961). Reactions to perceptual isolation and Rorschach manifestations of the primary process. W W. 2.5. 287-302. Goldfried, M.R., Strickler, G., & Weiner, LB. (1971). Mb hWMW 319161006 Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Gorlow, L., Zimet, C. N., & Fine, H. H. (1952). The validity of anxiety and hostility Rorschach content scores among adolescents. W 1.6.. 73-75- Guilford, J.P., s. Fruchter, B. (1973). W W (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Guntrip. H. (1969). WWW self. New York: International Universitites Press. Halpern, F. (1940). Rorschach interpretation of the personality structure of schizophrenics who benefit from insulin therapy. W 1.4. 826-833- Hayden, B. (1981). Rorschach Cards IV and VII revisited. W W. :15. 226-229- Henmendinger, L. (1953). Perceptual organization and development as reflected in the structure of Rorschach test responses. Journal www.11. 162-170. Hertz, M.R. (1943). Personality patterns in adolescence as portrayed by the Rorschach ink-blot method: IV the 'Erlebnistypus." W 22. 3-45. Hertz, M.R. (1951). Current problems in Rorschach theory and technique- WW5.15. 307-338- 91 Hollingshead, A.B., & Redlich, F.C. (1958). W illness. New York: Wiley. Holt, R.R. (1956). Gauging primary and secondary processes in Rorschach reSPonseS- MW. 211. 14-25. Kallstedt, F. E. (1952). A Rorschach study of sixty-six adolescents. WW 3.. 129-132- Karon, B.P. (1963). 'Ihe resolution of acute schizophrenic reactions: A contribution to the development of nonclassical psychotherapeutic techniques. W 1, 27-43. Karon, B.P., & Rosberg, J. (1958). Study of the mother-child relationship in a case of paranoid schizophrenia. Amiga W. 12. 522-533 . Kierkegaard, S. (1846). W. NewYork: Harper 5. Row, 1938. Klopfer, B., Ainsworth, M.D., Klopfer, W. G., 8 Holt,” R.R. (1954). m. Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World Book Company. 5 Klopfer, B., Ainsworth, M.D., Klopfer, W.G., & Holt, R.R. (1956). DJ l'llEllll' 11.13.”: W. Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World Book Company. Ledwith, N. (1959). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press. Levine, N., Glass, Melzoff. (1959). Toe inhibition process, Rorschach movement responses and intelligence: Sane further data. m W41. 306-312. Levitt, E., & Truumaa, A. (1972). ' ' W. New York: Grune and Stratton. Lidz. T. et a1. (1965). W- New York: International Universities Press. L162. T. (1973). MW New York: Basic Books. Lu, Y. (1961). Mother-child role relations in schizophrenia. Zambian. 2.4. 133-142- Mahler. Iii-8. (1975). W- London: Hutchinson and Company. Masterson. J. (1976). W. New York: Bruner/Mazel. 92 McNemar, Q.L. (1962). W (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley. Mednick, S., & Schulsinger, F. (1970). Factors related to breakdown in children at high risk for schizophrenia. In M. Roff, s. D. Ricks (Eds ). WW (Vol- 1) Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Meile-Dworetzki, G. (1956). 'Ihe development of perception in the Rorschach. In B. Klopfer (195.), W W (pp. 104-176). New York: World Book. Melnick, B., & Hurley, J. R. (1969). Distinctive personality attributes of child-abusing mothers. WW 91W. 33. 746-749. Meyer, R.G., & Karon, B.P. (1967). The schizophrenic mother concept and the 'IAT. W, 31),, 173—179. Mitchell, K.M. (1968) . An analysis of the schizophrenic mother concept by means of the Thematic Apperception Test. W WM. 11. 6. 571-574. Mitchell, K.M. (1969a). Concept of ”pathogenesis" in parents of schizophrenic and normal children. W W. 14.. 4. 423-424. Mitchell, K.M. (1969b). Social class and the schizophrenic mother concept- W. 2.4.. 463-469. MitChellp Lo". (1971). I! . I W Unwblished manuscript, University of Georgia, Athens, GA. Miner. J.B. (1957). W NewYork: Wiley. Mogar, R.E. (1962). Anxiety indices in human figure drawings: A replication and extension. MW. 25. 108. Murray. EA. (1943). W51 Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press. Neiger, S., Slemon, A.G., & Quirk, D.A. (1965). Rorschach scales of regression in psychosis. 555W. .71. Nicoles, N. (1970) . offspring. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan. 93 Ogdon, D. P., 8 Allee, R. (1959). Rorschach relationships with intelligence among familial mental defectives. W W. 53. 889-896. Parens, H., 8 Saul, L.J. (1971). W. New York: International Universities Press. Paulsen, A. (1941). Rorschachs of school beginners. Rorschach W. 5. 24-29. Piotrowski, z. (1939). Rorschach manifestations of improvement in insulin treated schizophrenics. 2W. 1. 508-526. Piotrowski, 2., 8 Schreiber, M. (1952). Rorschach perceptanalytic measurement of personality changes during and after intensive psychoanalytically oriented psychotherapy. In G. Bychowski8 JoL- Deepert (Eds). WW (pp- 337-361). New York: Basic Books. Pitman, E.J. (1937). Significance tests which may be applied to samples from any popalation, II. The correlation coefficient test. WW .4. 225-233 . Potkay, C. (1971). MW. NewYork: Gruneand Stratton. Rabin, A.I. (1960). Projective methods and projection in children. In A.I. Rabin 8 MR. Haworth (11:18.).me children (pp. 2-10). New York: Grune and Stratton. Rabin, A.I., 8 Beck, S.J. (1950). Genetic aspects of some Rorschach factors. Waist. 29. 595-599. Rapaport, D. (1952) . Projective techniques and the theory of techniques Wishing. 16.. 269-275. Rapaport, D., Gill, M., 8 Schafer, R. (1946). W Wearing (Vol. 2). Chicago: Yearbook Publishers. Rorschach, H. (1921). W. Bern 8 Leipzig: Ernst Bircher Verlag. Sanders, R., 8 Cleveland, S. E. (1953). The relationship between certain examiner personality variables and subjects' Rorschach scores. W 11. 34-50- Schafer. R. (1954). WWW rearing. New York: Grune and Stratton. Schulman, I. (1953). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University. 94 Siegel, E.L. (1953). Genetic parallels of perceptual structuralization in paranoid schizophrenia: An analysis by means of the Rorschach technique. W 189111119125. 11. 151-161- Siegel, S.M. (1956). The relationship of hostility to authoritarianism- MW 52. 368-372. Snith, J. R., 8 Coleman, J. C. (1956). The relationship between manifestations of hostility in projective tests and overt behavior. MW 29.. 326-334 Saner, R. (1958). Assaultiveness and two types of Rorschach color responses. W. 2.2. 57-62. Stotsky, B.A. (1952). A comparison of remitting and nonranitting schizophrenics on psychological testS- W mm. .41. 489-496. Tanaka, F. (1958). Rorschach movement responses in relation to intelligence. WW 5. 85-91. 'Ihetford, W. N., Molish, H. B., 8 Beck, S.J. (1951). Developmental aspects of personality structure in normal children. M p 15, 58-780 VandenBos, G.R., 8 Karon, B.P. (1971). Pathogenesis: A new therapist personality dimension related to therapeutic effectiveness. W 3.5. 252-260- Weiner. LB. (1966). MW NewYork: Wiley. Westrope, M.R. (1953). Relations among Rorschach indices, manifest anxiety, and performance under stress. W Wm.18. 515-524. Francis-Williams, J. (1968). W. London: Pergamon Press. Woody. R. (1981). W San Fransisco: J ossey-Bass. Wynne, L.C., 8 Singer, IM.T. (1963). 'Ihought disorder and family relations of schizophrenics: I. A research strategy. arching Winn. 2. 191-198. Wynne, L. C., 8 Singer, M.T. (1963). Thought disorders and family relations of schizophrenics: II. A classification of forms of thinking- WW 9.. 199-206- 95 HICH IGQN 312 STRTE UNIV. LIBRRRIES IIIHIIHIIHIHIHUIIHIIIINHIIHIHIIIIIHHI 93010632028