77 .2» :III} 1.7,, I7, II JIIIII II‘I‘IIIN'II III‘II . . "5' 73777777 ”“6; ”qu1 I .-7 ., ‘ -.. ~-‘ . h - u r 45‘ f. -- N‘af .‘.~ \ ~ £2 -.., -. ..., 3’ 71 {I73 IIII' 3.7. I.. 3%: I 7-III III" 7:777}! III": flIIIIi I HI_‘I11'I ., III, , 77777777777777.32717777?’ ‘. IIW i‘ . “III... II‘III 'IfiII'u II ‘7'”, ‘ 7111" WI III“; é“: 7'7":f 7I‘l771'7’7 'l:' r7.. Ir “r177", HI: JIIV'T ‘ 7II }\II ”III 5 Hr"); {07' 1| I" .It ”7 I 7 7 .3 '17 .IIS‘C: "flrl‘ul" 771‘7‘ 33.!" . 'III' ' 1"”;3 "WU!" h 'r. ... IA'II'IW "’I.‘;.' . .- 9w H... 133.. 73:}; I "7;'- I?‘ 77.9 43“. R ‘F);“A;Or'.1'i‘¥l' 7‘77. {II 7 4‘77'7 ' - I ”I; . ..2 H I' 7.2:. I ‘1‘}! \‘J‘ira‘ Wu". L'I' 0' ;|"’:' \ "l' I7 I _ I‘¢I I‘J ’gfigfi III“ (”77777171771777.6777 7 I‘ .7“ :2‘ ‘ A \“ “V . K “‘3 '31" In, ”II/£77 I7,,' '77'7'7 "III‘III rIIIwI/ 7J7“ ;_.'III7. 77777777 'II'III-H” """I‘5" ‘ ‘- I I I .‘ 7-,I‘f.",I.‘: '.:'.,‘|- "7‘ I'/‘ ,‘7‘ '17 7"»':.'7. :7I7u7 w . ‘ 77' 7 . I -. 7 _ .17. , .‘ ':.7 777:“. _ 7 . “'7“ 1"" 17" -: "IHEIJIV “ “'1'! "If . . .. M '. IL I '- TZ,“ 7.77.71. ’ I. In'I-‘I‘ Ii"? ' .II-j I I77 \I'.'I7'." " "I‘uv 71;?W“.~;I I‘ ,. _. ., -- - .. I 7'} I; 270', ‘III 7' WE. ‘77” 'I III I -' 7777777777. “777777.77 Q1 a {NIP I (I I I II. '0 .iff,” 71.77 77777 7 77771777777 Ukuu‘ (7'7! 7.777J3'.,777’.777 I73" {I '73: IIIIIIIMM I-‘I‘77' I77”; Iuj’ “EJIIK‘ (to. :g‘ w?” “fi‘v—so _ A -7 - -~ ter-n- .. €E_ ._ ‘. ‘ -<-s ‘ . o. '. . ‘ . O ‘— . ”5“,- I I . KI. ’ I I ‘7‘1‘ ” 7777.7 77 77 77 7 7 7 ...II . {77%; g I..', I777 ._ ‘7II77I;7'7., {7.7 .I77 7.7; ,7, . I. ,7. “7,. ”:17!“ 471.777. I57I77II7I77, 7.77.777. 77777777 .7 77 'I'r 7710,77 II 7776 7.77,. :77 I ,7. ' .777I7,7II7 (7' IIII 7., . 77. 7 .7 .7 “7 7" f: , 7‘7. 7,7,7. 7‘ 7,77 “7777:: ’Ilh‘ '_7‘7";.- 473‘, .7 ‘7'I7I,I7I'.II7"I.‘,".‘7-'.3,712.177II‘I'I' .7. 73' i7.'7I.°, 71;?“ ' I 7 ."f 3'.-'5,‘7‘.’I;IIII'III7,‘IIII7'I-I‘77'1‘; 7o '7'17' 7'7.“ ' . t ‘ ' 1"! I III‘I'IE. 717 "I I: h‘ ‘3‘ka 7'7' 1'. I‘" If“ . ‘ .I‘QII.“.;, I j.I'7I:!I ’38:! II {NIH It” "'IIIII I I 1 “ :I:.'."J.:I.II’.":' WI)“ " ‘I‘J" II‘IEIE u; ' ’I' II}; 7vI ” SI.“ WM IaIQI’IE’n I I'EIII“."'.I‘ Ifftg' .37, .'.u”.’.. 777770737, 77.777777 7'777 77' 7 .7. [077‘ .777” 77, ,7'7777. _- 7477,7773 (“I7 . 7,- 777}, "I' 3‘77"“ .SIIw: . '3 ‘III'I I7II-77I'7I ~“' I;7Iit:’;"-.'I‘.:"~(' "r'f' 7 _7.7,7_.77 “77.7777 771:)”: 77 7777777777 (iv: Jr~77l’.{ rI' 7::7‘I’ .77II".777 I7. 7! I .\ .7?- 7.7377777777777777 47‘ II ..N‘I' ,‘IyHI'II.I-‘I'.JI“I"‘:CI"II‘I" [77.4717 7v7-' II M \‘J‘ _ _ - I 7'? I . . $777,777.77]. 7‘77: ‘77'II7'7‘77'7I'II’I‘7'77737-‘io:'I-.."III J7I7HIJ77I. II“.-'...I7...I-I IIIN: ‘I "'7 H6577 77 [I 7 I'II.‘ “NH/I I "II In?” ..7' “3ng 'M Ir'7.~"3[5.glIIIIJI7:I . I» .‘II “I. "‘II'I 7;, 'I ’é‘II’ I"\‘. 'I': I”! .3: I' ‘ ”"I II7I'I‘I I. “ ,1777717‘77 II‘: '9'I.7 ’1: I Iué7 . ' .7717. 7'.'.-.-':‘7r n7!“ .I'JEI777.‘ U777. . IIII-«II. ... W ‘: ':-.’. ...,I . :I_. 77" I77.’7." ‘.r .' .-;- ‘.‘,".."‘ .3 . l- 7I '7"I . 'I M 7, ‘I 'l h (I: ’I""7'J.r . ‘ I I' 'I‘7II “' ‘2. I I7 P..”.:', 1"I .II I' I" H 7‘": ”‘75 .' """ .' . '>‘I .‘ “". w.“ ‘1 ‘ .‘J‘. .7. . 77,777,777. 7 J II7 .‘I7‘, - 74, 7:7777 «7.77”... .. ,7-II7 .‘7, I7. .77., 7777,77. »:7II7,77,.'7I77I7.7:77{I7-..I77:777 . 7777" 7 I .m, 'I: NIH-5‘ ‘773I'7'7A ‘9 '7‘.’7'7.'x‘.- :3. 35793:“ my. . : ’ ,’;‘l', '.77'/ f 77.3.7 7 ‘0' 7973771777 77.771757“ I 7“,, IJ77UI7Iyg7lI‘I- A'AI‘III WV .".~‘I-II,“:' I7~'7. ..I In I‘ " H‘In‘w I ‘ I I ’77 7777 7; ~.(77.77(.77. 7773.71ti I] ’7‘ I7} imp-7&7? 7:71:17“ 77:7, 7 . I I I7": ‘: 7"7I ‘ .7 I] .~ 7..._7777I‘ " 77 7I7I77I’777177‘n7'777w “€41,777 .7 . 7. 77.7 ".""~-'."I-7-I". II III I,7I1l .71.,77-77. .’ ' ’7 .IIIIQll JIQ’M': aNT [7771' :77,I 3' (If 777,7’777'77; 777.7777 P 77)}: 7.7, 77.72.77 ‘4' -_-‘.~. x 71"“. '7': T] ‘7l7'l I] ”"7” I 7"" .77 7,5727 I777 “‘I‘lfl‘ I7 II 71?»)..7 I37)“; EFL 7H, 7.; “#74; G 07717 "~I‘..': III! ”I” 7777:7717" ‘flfi’g, '77:? ”Y 1“8077" III. ”"1“," ”(...-773:5“; . I: -III7-:us! 7.. '7IIFII‘I;7*7:E' I I b?“ .7. -, ., 'I‘I’I" ‘.‘.'III:.' IIIII.""t.._. '«II'ITJJIII'I‘J‘ ...:IIIIII ‘ "' III’I'. {IMMIIJinIzsmIz . ;. u u .‘ I \I-]«;. I . ,.' 5.0!! ~,‘ ."7 ‘>‘ ‘ - I ‘3. E V ‘ O WWWWWWWW WWWWWWWWWWWWWWW 31293 01063 2184 I LIBRARY ’ Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled FAMILY ENVIRDNMENT (DRREIATES OF AUDIESCENT DRDIKINGANDWSDRIVII‘E: AS'I'UDYOFTHE PRIZESSFS OF SEPARATIONAND INDIVIIIJATION presented by Cheryl Smith—Winberry has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph 0 Do degree in Psycmlogy %¢fl/ffi:{;/ Major professor/ flJohn Paul McKinney Date 1/ 23/87 MSU is an Alfirman'w- Action/Equal Opportunity Institution O~ 12771 MSU LIBRARIES . RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. LEA! 0 7 ‘9; I‘ FAMILY MW COW OF AmLEmI‘ DRINKIm AND REEKLESS DRIVII‘G: A STUDY OF THE PRCXIESSES OF SEPARATION AND INDIVIDUATION By Cheryl Smith-Winberry A DISSERTATIG‘J Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfil lment of the requirenents for the degree of IIXITOR OF PHILDSDPHY Department of Psychology 1987 ABSTRACT FAMILY ENVIRONMENT OORRELATES OF ADOLESCENT DRINKING AND RECKLESS DRIVING: A STUDY OF THE PROCESSES»OF SEPARATION’AND INDIVIDUATION By Cheryl Lyn Smith-Winberry This study examined the influences of family climate on the adolescent processes of separation and individuation. It was hypothesized that adolescents' management of alcohol consumption and driving practices, both rites of passage in American society, would be reflective of the manner in which they were separating from their families. School involvement and social environment were thought to mediate these relationships. Hypotheses were examined utilizing a questionnaire methodology with 244 junior and senior high school licensed drivers. Several variables were examined: (1) family climate-Family Environment Scale (Moos, 1974), (2) quantity/frequency drinking index, (3) problem drinking index, (4) transition to drinking index, (5) risky driving index--e.g. , accidents, moving violations, and close calls, (6) school comnitment-e.g., GPA, truancy, academic expec- tations, and (7) social envirorment influences—mg. , peer and parental support for and modeling of exaggerated drinking and driving practices . The results indicate that family environment variables are related to adolescent drinking and driving practices , even after accounting for peer influences. The stronger relationship between driving practices and family environment might suggest that family influences are more important.when initial learning takes place since drinking usually occurs before driving. Family environment further served to differentiate between normative and problematic drinking and driving. Specifically, a constellation of factors termed “family disregard” were present to a varying degree within the at-risk groups. High rates of drinking and risky driving were associated with.a social environment that approved of deviance and with school failure among adolescents. It was speculated that these families failed to provide their adolescents with appropriate values, monitoring, and a commitment to the larger community. In contrast, adolescents who engaged in normative drinking practices perceived their families to be moderately cohesive, more individuated, and tolerant of conflict. Abstainers were closely tied to their families which they perceived as less conflicted, less individuated, and either very cohesive or very disengaged. Perhaps moderate cohesion is a prerequisite for the process of separation; however, more successful adolescents also receive continued monitoring and guidance. Their families are less approving of exaggerated drinking and driving, and they have access to other sources of socially sanctioned adult status--e.g., school competency. To my husband, Larry, my children, Heather and Gabriel, and my parents. My family is the core through which all experiences and accomplishments become meaningful. iv AW I would like to extend my sincere appreciation to my committee members-QJ.P. McKinney, Ph.D., R.A. Zucker, Ph.D., R. Frankmann, Ph.D., Department of Psychology, and D. Smith, Ph.D., Department of Highway Traffic Safety. Their individual expertise and combined guidance enriched the quality of the thesis. I am particularly grateful to two members who have influenced my professional development. Jehn P. McKinney's commitment to teaching was outstanding. He provided me with the opportunity to be a part of a researCh team where I was able to acquire skills from a competent group of individuals. Moreover, I truly appreciate the many hours we spent discussing issues relevant to professional and personal development. Robert A. Zucker has provided excellent clinical and research guidance throughout my graduate training; however, I am indebted to him for a more significant reason: he provided me with a role model of competent professionalism. Bob offered sound guidance in a manner that increased my awareness of professional ethics, respected my individuality, and supported my strengths. Additionally, I would like to thank my family and friends for their support. My husband, Larry Winberry, provided timely support and abundant tolerance as I progressed through this work. my mother, Janet.Weathers', unwavering confidence in my abilities allowed me to persevere during times of personal doubt. Finally, my friends were always available for consultation and support. Special thanks to JUdy Larsen, Ph.D., for skillfully providing editorial assistance and Suzy Pavick for adding the final touches. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF THESOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOO LIST OF FIGLJRESOOOOCOOOOOOOOOOOOOO............OOOOOOOOIOOOO WWIWC......-.....OOOOO00.0.00.........OOOOOOOOOOOOO Theories of Adolescent Socio~emotional Development.... Family Climate Correlates of Adolescent Separation and Individuation..................... Adolescent Driving and Family Correlates.............. Adolescent Drinking and Family Correlates............. Drinking and Reckless Driving: common or Unique Problems?................................. Application of NGw’Terminology........................ Significance of This Investigation.................... Hypotheses............................................ WWOOOOOOIOOOOCOOO00.0.0000.............OOOOOOOOOOOOCOO SllbjectS.O.............OOOOOOOOOIOOOOOOOO00.00.0000... Prmures.......OOOOOOOOOCOOCOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO masureSooooooooocoo-o.000.000.0000....00.000.000.000. ClaSSification Of S‘ijects.........OOOOOOOOOOOOOOIOOOO RESULTS.O.............OOOOOOOOOOOOOCOOOO......OOOIOOOOOOOOO Personal Subject Characteristics of Sample............ Family Environment, Drinking, and Driving Scale Reliabilities.............................. Testing the Hypothesized Curvilinear Relationships.... Overall Relationship Between Family Environment and Adolescent Drinking and Risky Driving........ Hypothesis #1 (Family Environment and Risky'Driving).............................. Hypothesis #4 (Family Environment and Drinking).. Hypothesis #7 (Family Environment and the Transition to Drinking...................... Relative Relationship of Peer and Family Environment Influences........................... Hypothesis #2 (Peer versus Family Environment-Driving)....................... vii Page xi 10 37 50 52 53 S6 62 62 62 ‘63 7O 73 73 76 80 84 91 93 95 99 99 Hypothesis #5 (Peer versus Family Environment-Drinking)...................... Hypothesis #8 (Peer versus Family Environment—-Transition to Drinking)........ School, Peer, and Parental Correlates................. Hypothesis #3 (Correlates of Adolescent Risky'Driving).............................. Hypothesis #6 (Correlates of Adolescent Drinking)................................... Hypothesis #9 (Correlates of the Transition to Drinking)................................... DIWSSIW.o0000000000000000000000.0000...ooooooooooooooooo sum.......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.00000.........OOOCOOOOOOCOOO ”MIX AOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO...O WSOIOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOCO... viii 104 108 109 111 111 113 115 135 140 150 IJST OF TABLES Table Page 1a Descriptive Information on Sample Characteristics......... 74 1b Chi Square Analyses of the Driving Groups by Sex, COhtrOlla for me.0.0.0.000.........OOOOOOOO0.0.0.0000... 75 1c Chi Square Analyses of the Drinking Groups by Sex, C0ntr011fi for Age.O.......OOOOOOCOOOOOOO0.0.0.0...0...... 77 1d Percentages of Subjects Within Each Drinking Category-2A.Comparison to the Zucker and Hartford (1984) ijjngSOOOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOO 78 2 Internal consistency Scores for Scaled variables on Measures of Family Environment, Driving, Drinking and mer supmrt.0.0.000.......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.00.00.00.00 79 3 Correlations of the Curvilinear Association Between Cohesion, Expressiveness, Control & Organization and Drinking & Driving After Partialling Out the linear Associations....................................... 83 4 Intercorrelations of Family Environment Scales............ 86 5 Intercorrelations of the Drinking and Driving masues.O.........O.................OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO... 87 6 Multiple Regression Analysis of the Family. mnomnt mlesaIflRiSKy DI-iVi-IEOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO.... 88 7 canonical Analysis for the Family Environment Scales am tm Drinking masureSOO0.0.............OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 89 8 Multiple Regression Analysis of Family Environment mtmTranSitiontODriMing ImeXOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 90 9 Correlations Between the Family Environment Scales WRiSky DriVj-rg.0.0.0.0..........OOOOOOOOOO......OOOOOOO 92 10 Correlations of the Sex x Family Environment Inter~ actions With Driving After the Main Effects Have w Partia11$ mtOO..........00............OOOOIOOOOOOOO 94 ll 14 15 l6 17 18 19 20 Table 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Correlations Between the Family Environment Scales am ”E Drirlking masureSOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOI....0... Correlations of the Sex x Family Environment Inter- actions With Drinking After the Main Effects Have m Partialla mtOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOI......OOOOOOOOOOOO... Correlations Between the Family Environment Scales arfl tm TraDSj-tion to DrinkmgOOOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Correlations of the Sex x Family Environment Inter- actions With Transition to Drinking After the Main EffeCtS I-Iave m Partialla mtOOOOO......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO A Mixed Hierarchical-Stepwise Regression Analysis, Examining the Correlations of Family Environment and Driving After Peer Support and Modeling Have Been Accounted For........................................ A Mixed Hierarchical-Stepwise Regression Analysis, Examining the Correlations of Family Environment and Drinking After Peer Support and Modeling Have Been Accounted For: Entire Sample......................... A Mixed Hierarchical-Stepwise Regression Analysis, Examining the Correlations of Family Environment and Drinking After Peer Support and Modeling Have Been Accounted For: Cross validation #1................... A Mixed Hierarchical-Stepwise Regression Analysis, Examining the Correlations of Family Environment and Drinking After Peer Support and Modeling Have Been Accounted For: Cross validation #2................... A Mixed Hierarchical-Stepwise Regression Analysis, Examining the Correlations of Family Environment and the Transition to Drinking Index After Peer Support and Modeling Have Been Accounted For.............. Correlations Between School, Peer, and Parent Measures and the Drinking and Driving Measures..................... Page 96 97 98 100 102 105 106 107 110 112 10 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Circumplex model: Sixteen types of marital and fmily SYStaNSooococo-0.0000000cooooooooooooooooooooooooooo 16 2 A heuristic model for changes in influencing structures affecting behavior over developmental time...... 27 3 An organizational structure for classes of influences umn drivm WVior-oon.on000.000.000.00oooooooooooooooooo 28 4 An organizational structure for classes of influences upon drinking mmvj-oroo0.0.0.0.0.00.00........OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 29 5 Defining criteria for adolescent drinking levels, and percentages of respondents in each categoryb- by sex and age and for total sample........................ 38 6 variables examined by Jessor & Jessor (1978) in their study of adolescent transition to alcohol use........ 46 7 Family environment scale subscale descriptions............. 64 8 Internal consistencies, average item-subscale cor- ‘ relations and test-retest reliabilities for FES............ 65 9 Means and standard deviations of FES short form SlulbscaleSOI0.0...00......0.00.0000.........OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 67 10 Illustration of a significant relationship between Cd‘eSj-on am riSky driVj-rg.0.0.0.0.........OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 83 xi WHEN The hypothesis that a family's social climate affects the course of child development and behavioral expression has influenced theoreticians and researchers since the 18th century. Although the importance of the family environment is rarely questioned, systematic attempts to identify, measure, and relate the essential elements of the environment to specific areas of child development or to particular behavioral outcomes is a more recent endeavor . Early attempts at classifying family environment focused on unitary constructs, such as authoritarianism versus permissiveness in parental management styles (Lewin, Lippitt & White, 1939; Elder, 1963; Baumrind, 1967) . Further, many of the studies focused on an age group in which family influences were thought to be more salient governors of development--i.e. , infancy through early childhood. One reason for such a beginning in the area of family research is the realization that ”the family” is a very complex system which changes continuously both to meet the demands of its individual members, and to find its niche or homeostasis within the larger social milieu—Le” the community. Any attarpt at quantification of such an evolving entity is replete with theoretical and methodological difficulties. Thus, in an effort to begin to piece together the puzzle, carplexities are simplified, and extrasystem influences are momentarily ignored . 2 More recently, a number of investigators have attempted to capture the essence of the family social climate, examining multiple constructs thought to be important governors of interpersonal behavior and applying such models to the entire span of the family life cycle (Olson & McCubbin, 1983). Thus, not only is the family examined at times of relative closure-i.e., when extrafamilial influences are minimal--but it is also examined during periods of rapid change and reformation-i.e., during adolescence when extrafamilial influences rise as the family prepares for the exiting of one of its members. Further, various qualities of the family environment are being related to individual functioning and behavior in many areas of develop- ment--e.g., social, cognitive, and emotional (Dickinson, Hess, Miyake & Azuma, 1979; wynne & Cole, 1983; Hess, Azuma, Kashiwag, Dickinson & Nagano, 1984). The purpose of this study is to examine the influences of the family social climate on the normal adolescent processes of separation and individuation. Adolescents' perceptions of ten constructs of their family environment-cohesion, conflict, expressiveness, achievement orientation, intellectual-cultural orientation, independence, moral-religious orientation, active-recreation, organization, and control-awhich fall into three general dimensions-~i.e., interpersonal relationships among family members, directions of personal growth, and organizational structures-dwere assessed in a nonclinical, high school population of juniors and seniors (N:244). These perceptions were related to two areas of behavior thought to be reflective of impending adult status, behaviors commonly referred to as rites of passage in our society: (1) adolescent alcohol consumption, and (2) adolescent 3 driving. Further, in an effort to understand the complexity of these associations, nonfamily influences were assessed and incorporated in the model. It is anticipated that such a research endeavor will provide useful information on an important and frequently neglected aspect of drinking, driving, and the combination of the two; namely, family influences. However, this is not a model of pathology. It is a model which deals with the spectrum of possible outcomes in two defined areas as they relate to various styles of family functioning. The emphasis here is on understanding the interdependence of the family process and individual adolescent growth, not in explaining the deviant expressions of either process. The need for understanding family influences upon adolescent functioning appears to be a point of debate within the scientific community. Developmentally, there seems to be agreement that early interactions affect many aspects of later functioning. During adolescence, youngsters are seen as turning from their parents as a frame of reference to adopt the standards of their peers (Sullivan, 1953). The assumption is that at this point, family influences assume a subservient role in comparison to peer and other extrafamilial influences. In 1963, Brittain subjected this traditional view to empirical evaluation. The study focused on the issue of the relative influence of parents and peers on adolescents' choices. Briefly, a large number of high school girls were asked to respond to a hypothetical situation after hearing advice from both a peer and a parent. For the experimental group, the referent was changed between the first and second administration so that the opinion of a peer at time A was the 4 opinion of the parent at time B. The control group received either parental or peer opinions during both administrations . The investigators discovered that the control subjects were more consistent in accepting the advice of the referent they were presented with-either parent or peer. However, the experimental subjects were more likely to vary their opinion depending on the situation. For less significant problems they would rely on the advise of the peer group. In contrast, for more intimate problems, they are more likely to turn to their Regent . Since this study, a large body of literature has accumulated on the relative influences of the family versus the peer group during adolescence. An inspection of the empirical findings leads one to the conclusion that gin systems are important. Investigations that put forth a polarized view of the issue, suggesting that the family has little influence or that the family is the only impactful system, are overlooking the complexity of the adolescent's social world. Unfortunately, family influences continue to be given a lower priority in adolescent research, compared to biological , peer and societal influences. The importance of pursuing such a line of research becomes clear when one considers the significant impact the family has on the adolescent's successful accomplishment of age-appropriate social—emotional tasks (Glueck & Glueck, 1950; Haley, 1980) . Prior to the presentation of the actual study, a review of the existing family research dealing with the adolescent's expression of the age-appropriate need for separation and individuation wil 1 be made . The focus will be on the aforementioned ten dimensions of family climate and how these dimensions relate to adolescent emotional and 5 behavioral functioning. As a background for this review, various theories of adolescent socio—emoticnal development will be presented in order to provide a foundation for understanding the tasks*with.which a family with.an adolescent Child is confronted. Finally, the rationale for choosing drinking and driving as the behavioral areas to examine further the relationship between family climate and adolescent development‘will be discussed. What is known about the phenomenology of drinking and driving will be summarized separately, emphasizing the family correlates. Theories of Adolescent Socio—emotional Development Adolescence has been described as the period in the life cycle during which the establishment of a personal identity is of paramount importance (Erikson, 1959). Identity formation has been explored through research in a variety of areas: ego identity, identity formation, identity achievement, identity status, and identity crisis. At the basis of all of these concepts is the idea that the adolescent strives to develop an inner organization of his/her needs, values, and attitudes. The task is to come to terms with.one's own uniqueness as a person. The more solid the development of personal identity, the more likely the individual is to be able to resist outside temptations to conform to social pressures. James E. Marcia (1980) describes identity as an "existential position, to an inner organization of needs, abilities, and self-perceptions as well as to a sociopolitical stance." According to his formulation of identity, the adolescent is striving to come to terms with his/her own individuality, while at the same time learning to view himself/herself in relation to external social structures. Failure to resolve these identity issues may lead to 6 "identity diffusion”, which describes a state of feeling empty or insignificant. This situation has been linked to various conditions characterized by a withdrawal from reality or by personality disintegration (Erikson, 1968). In many respects, this theory is the intrapsychic equivalent of Durkheim's theory of social anomiet Erik Erikson (1959, 1963, 1968) has been the most influential writer on identity development. He notes that identity development is a psychosocial task distinctive of, but not exclusive to, adolescence. In his theory of psychosocial development, Erikson describes the tasks at various points of development in terms of crises-~transition points in which decisions about one's life situation and personal values must be made. During adolescence, the crisis is identity versus diffusion. According to the theory, the search for identity does not begin in adolescence. Rather, it.begins as self-object differentiation at infancy and reaches its final stage with the self-mankind integration at old age. What is unique about adolescent effbrts toward identity integration is that "this is the first time cognitive skills, social expectations, and physical development coincide to enable young persons to sort through and synthesize their childhood identifications in order to construct a viable pathway toward their adulthood" (Marcia, 1980). McKinney et al. (1982) note that it is not the passive acceptance of ascribed roles, such as American, scholar, Jew or Black which are earmarks of a successful identity resolution. Rather, the individual must be active in choosing between these alternatives; "the individual must be an agent as well as a patient." The resolution of developmental crises is characterized by a public commitment to a set of life expectations and personal values. 7 In making Erikson's formulation empirically more explicit, Marcia (1980) defined four distinct identity types-erole diffusion, foreclosure , moratorium, and achievement of a mature ego identity—which he believes reflect possible outcomes of an adolescent's struggle with identity issues. He postulates two dimensions which distinguish among the four statuses: the degree of prior struggles with.an identity crisis and the degree of commitment to a system of values. In role diffusion, individuals experience no internal anxiety because they neither challenge ascribed values nor commit themselves to the values with which they have been presented. Fbreclosure is an adaptation in which personal struggle is kept to a minimal level by an acceptance of ascribed values which are presented to the child during development, usually by a parent. Accordingly, sophisticated cognitive skills which can assimilate present and past experiences and accommodate one's belief structures are not employed since the process of an active search for self is avoided. Moratorium describes individuals who are struggling to achieve an independent identity; however, the struggle itself, rather than the final resolution, becomes the focus. SuCh individuals are unwilling to make a commitment to personal values. Instead, they seem to rebel against all values which they feel are being imposed upon them. Finally, a mature ego identity is achieved when the moratorium crisis is successfully resolved. A personal commitment to political, religious, vocational, and sexual goals and values has been made. Empirical efforts to quantify and study "identity" began with studies of the validity of the construct. Subsequently, many personality correlates have been investigated-i.e., anxiety, 8 self-esteem, authoritarianism, moral reasoning and cognition. What seems clear is that there is abundant evidence that identity issues are a governing aspect of adolescent development, that youngsters can be distinguished in terms of their involvement in identity struggles, and that resolution of identity struggles--e.g., a mature commitment to a set of values-is a relative concept. That is, a homeostasis is reached and the individual adopts a new sense of self which continues to be modified throughout the adult years. The aforementioned struggles in forming an independent identity have frequently been interpreted as indicative of a period of development replete with stress. G. Stanley Hall viewed the adolescent stage of development in this manner; he adopted the term Sturm und Drang-storm and stress. This view continues to have its proponents, particularly among sociologists and psychoanalytically oriented psychologists (McKinney, Fitzgerald, & Strommen, 1980). Some writers have gone so far as to characterize all adolescents as possessing a psychopathology (Goldstein, 1971). Put to the test of scientific rigor, this view of adolescence has not been supported. In a well-known longitudinal study of middle-class adolescent males from ages 14 to 22, Offer and Offer (1975) conclude that over the 8 years of development, turmoil is not the universal state of adolescence, nor is it a necessary component of healthy ego development. The study identified three groups of adolescents: continuous growth group, surgent growth group, and tumultuous growth group. TWithin each group, measures of social and emotional functioning were highly correlated. Thus, there is evidence for a range of ego development among adolescents that appears to be consistent within groups of adolescents. 9 A natural extension of the pathologyboriented view of adolescence is the notion that adolescent stress and rebellion will naturally spill over into the family. As the adolescent struggles to establish.his own identity, he must rebel against parental values and authority in order to complete the process. Bandura and.Walters (1959) examined these hypotheses in a normative sample of adolescents and their parents. They found that the boys had internalized the parents' values and standards of behavior to a large degree; consequently, restrictions and external controls had been lightened as the parents came to believe that their children could manage their own behavior. Further, these authors found that emancipation was not a stressful issue, but an issue that had been in process since preadolescence. Thus, there is no more evidence to support the view of family dystncti n during adolescence than there is to support the view of adolescent dysfunction. It seems logical to assume that families show as much diversity in their ability to cope with the added demands of adolescence as do the adolescents themselves. In summary, the task of forming an independent identity is posited to be a central component of adolescence. The family counterparts of this task are the processes of separation from close family ties and individuation. Inherent in these changes is some normal amount of stress and conflict. For the majority of families,the adolescents' needs are accommodated, with the family serving as a reliable foundation that provides the support needed to make an exit. For other families, meeting these normal developmental demands is more difficult. In part, this may be due to a greater degree of conflict within the adolescent. On the other hand, it could be due to a lack of coping 10 resources within another subsystem of the familyb-i.e., parents, marital dyad, family, etc. In an effort to sort through these possiblilties, it.would be helpful to take a closer look at specific family climate variables as they relate to the range of psychological and behavioral manifestations of the adolescent's attempt to separate and individuate. Family Climate Correlates of Adolescent Separation and Individuaticn Because the focus of this study is on normal variations in the social climate of families with adolescents, it seems useful to review the data on dimensions of family functioning which are hypothesized to be strengths. To this end, two sets of beliefs will be examined: (1) clinical interpretation of family strengths, and (2) individual family members' views of positive family attributes. Fisher and Sprenkle (1978) believed that a disproportionate amount of attention had been given to understanding family pathology; consequently, they developed a study to provide clinical practitioners ‘with information on the theoretical principles other colleagues used in strengthening family relationships. As a starting point, they reviewed the literature in three areas of family functioning: cohesion, adaptability, and commmication. Next, they developed a list of traits which are used to operationalize these theoretical constructs. An initial sample of 600 clinicians were asked to rank the seven most important features of healthy family functioning. Fifty-two percent of the clinicians responded to the survey. The highest ranked traits, in descending order, were attentive listening, value sender, flexibility, differentiation, speak for self, psychological safety, expression of feelings, support, negotiation, send congruent messages, and attention 11 to affect. The lowest ranked items were: express thoughts, indicate message is received, spontaneous speech, loyalty, feedback, report complete thoughts, paraphrase, and physical caretaking. The results of this survey indicate that clinicians view healthy families as able to create an environment where family members feel safe, supported, and valued. Of lesser importance are traits which focus on specialized interactional or communication techniques . Another study of family therapists found that cohesion, adaptability and communication dimensions offered important goals for treatment. The Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry (1970) asked family therapists to indicate their primary goals for therapy from a list of eight goals. Of the 290 respondents, 85% chose improved communication as primary; 56% chose improved empathy, 56% chose autonomy and individuation; 34% chose more flexible leadership; 23% chose reduced conflict. Individual symptomatic improvement was chosen by 23% and improved individual task performance by 12%. ‘Ihus, the emphasis is again on creating a supportive climate primarily through the strengthening of communication skills. Of lesser importance are system maintenance and personal growth dimensions . The second primary source of information on healthy family functioning consists of the opinions of individual family members. Otto (1963) solicited volunteers from the comrunity to attend ten one-and-a-half hour family discussion sessions. 'menty-seven couples from all stages of the life cycle participated in the project. The sessions were taped and transcribed. The fol lowing list contains the ten strengths which received the most attention and agreement from the group: 12 (1) The ability to provide for the physical, emotional, and spiritual needs of a family. (2) The ability to be sensitive to the needs of the family members. (3) The ability to communicate effectively. (4) The ability to provide support, security and encouragement. (5) The ability to initiate and maintain growth-producing relationships and experiences within and without the family. (6) The capacity to maintain and create constructive and responsible comunity relationships in the neighborhood, the school, tom and local government. (7) The ability to grow with and through children. (8) An ability for self-help, and the ability to accept help when appropriate. (9) An ability to perform family roles flexibly. (10) Mutual respect for the individuality of family members. In contrast to clinicians, it would appear that families put more of an emphasis on interpersonal climate variables. 01 the other hand, while viewing this dimension as primary, clinicians are more likely to place greater value on system maintenance aspects than families do. Further support for this observation cores from a study conducted by Fisher, Gibbin, and Hoopes (1982). They correlated nonclinical family members views' about the nature of a healthy family with family therapists' perceptions. Families ranked items associated with the dimensions of cohesion higher than therapists. m the other hand, therapists ranked flexibility and shared leadership-adaptability items--higher than families did. 13 The findings from such researdh efforts provide valuable data on "healthy family functioning." The need for establishing criteria for normalcy and strength becomes clear when one attempts to define and treat pathology. Hewever, there is a growing concern that broad definitions of family strengths are inadequate to capture the finer characteristics that distinguish.families at.different points in the family life cycle (Duvall, 1962; Rapoport, 1963; Hill, 1964; Olson & McCubbin, 1983). Thus, attempts are underway to divide the family life cycle into stages so that phase specific dynamics can be investigated. "Adolescence" is posited to be one stage in the family life cycle that requires its own unique skills and strengths in order to meet adequately the concomitant needs of the developing child and maturing parents. The literature on adolescent family development cores from two primary sources: (1) studies on representative, nonclinical families ‘with an adolescent child who is usually the oldest child, and (2) studies on clinical populations that attempt to distinguish between the families of healthy and disturbed adolescents. Of particular interest is research that isolates family environment variables during different phases of development, allowing for direct comparisons between phases. One research team succeeded in such a feat, giving special attention to the adolescent phase of development. The work of Olson and MoCubbin (1983), presented in Families, What Makes Them Wbrk?, will be reviewed. Additionally, selected research focusing on family correlates of adolescent functioning will be reviewed. The dimensions of interpersonal relationships, individual growth, and system maintenance will be emphasized. l4 Methodological considerations. Before beginning a discussion of the empirical findings, a brief mention of several methodological considerations regarding family researdh is needed. First, Jacob (1975) cogently argues for the need to present separate findings for each sex. Adolescent males and females exhibit different types of deviance, suggesting that different family factors may also be important. Secondly, the adolescent/family literature operationalizes the concepts of healthy versus unhealthy individual adolescent and family functioning in a wide variety of ways. This variation makes the process of direct comparison of studies difficult at best. A related prdblem concerns the measures which are employed in the studies. Frequently, the investigators choose to measure family concepts by developing a new scale or questionnaire. Although such procedures are not uncommon in the initial stages of researdh, they add to the complexity of between-study cotparisons. A fourth weakness lies in the simplistic definitions, albeit necessarily so, of family environment. As more information accumulates, complex theories of family process can be devised and tested through the application of sophisticated multivariate analytic techniques. Finally, sampling biases, particularly in researCh employing clinical populations, limits the overall generalizability of the findings. Based upon the current methodological inadequacies, there exists a need for research which (1) takes a family systems perspective, (2) utilizes established—~reliable and valid-instruments, (3) examines a representative sample of adolescents and their families, (4) employs several alternative definitions of healthy versus unhealthy functioning, and (5) considers sex differences when analyzing the data. 15 Empirical findings with normal adolescent families. Olson and {McCubbin (1983) sought to capture the complexity of marriage and family life across the life cycle. They developed a theoretical model, the circumplex model, which classifies families along two dimensions—~adaptability and cohesion. combining their relative position on each dimension, families are described as falling into one of 16 types. Figure 1 illustrates Olson et al.'s model. Balanced families, represented diagrammatically by the four center areas, are thought to possess the most resources for dealing with familial stress and developmental crises. Extreme family types, represented by the outside corners of the diagram, are characterized by a limited ability to meet familial and extrafamilial demands. Finally, midrange families, represented by the middle circle of the diagram, possess several strengths and resources, yet also manifest certain deficits which may present difficulties when the families are faced with specific types of stressors. The family life cycle was divided into seven stages of development--prechild, young child, school-aged child, adolescent, launching, empty nest, retirementv-based upon an expansion of Hill's work (1949). Olson and McCubbin utilized a survey methodology. A total of 2692 individuals from 31 states responded to the survey. The sample consisted of 1140 couples, plus 412 adolescents with.equal numbers of males and females. Each stage of the life cycle was represented by at least 100 families. The sample included mainly white, working-class families, with only 15% of parents lacking a high school education. Indices of family types, family resources, family stress and change, family coping strategies and marital/family satisfaction were examined. <——— Low Hugh —-9 UtSlWCIGIO 5!”le CONNlCHD ileSNlO CXIAINKABIP» CllhlfifiABIP'JIE ‘ v— V V . -‘7 ./ ’.//'/;/4///./.’N;7 A conessou tumour cuuucau r _ unsucactq -' 'LHAOHCALU /;'7W/;W}W7/ CHAONLM l v tthSnto tvf/, Hugh I A .o ' D ,. , A mum : mmu umau Mum Human P t‘ OISENGAGIO SIPARMIO CONNECIEO ' thMlSNlo T W/ / / / _ / I A W. 7/ I ’/ / ~ // ‘? 77/7f/r 7///// I | SlRuCluRlD ,suwciunmv smucwmuu smucwmuu 'smucwmuv T . DISINGAGIO SIPARAMD CONNECIEO . lNMlSNlO Y 1 I ,— c i RIGID” DISINGAGID Si PARA“ CONNKCIEO (WHISHIO V ;‘._mmnom .3 30: m m> 80503.59: mHUZEfiEmEXV az< moH>.05). An average of 35% of the subjects fell within the risky driving category. 73 74 Table 1a Descriptive Information on Sample Characteristics Characteristic Mean Mode Group Percentages (%) Age 16.70 17.00 16 yrs. (38) 17 yrs. (55) 18 yrs. (07) 20 yrs. (00) Grade --- 12.00 11th (44) 12th (56) Sex ——- Males Males (55) Females (45) Race —-- Caucasian Caucasian (97) Hispanic (01) Black (00) Native American (02) Asian (00) Parents ---- Married thrried (76) Marital Divorced (24) Status 75 mo. . m 58. u 063 numxm 98:3,... 8. Am ...a H fie. .no. u chosen who mo. A m ...E H fins .86 u «page... do mm.mm wm.eo wo.ov mw.>m wo.he am.vm H m x mm m 3 @836 baa mm.mo wm.mm wo.om mm.mo wo.mm wm.mm m m on we mm on mum>euo ommm moneom onmz mofimeom moan: mofimeom mofimz me u own now be u own now me u own new 08. 8: 83:8 no... @520ch son 9.: 35:8 woe mom ooHHonucoo .xmm an mmsouo.m:e>euo new no mwmwamc< cocoon ecu 0: 63mg 76 Drinking status was determined by theoretical classification schemes suggested by Zucker (1980) and Jessor et a1. (1974). Definitions of four categories of drinkers-i.e., abstainers, norma- tive-light, normative—moderate , and exaggerated—were based upon two criteria--quantity/frequency of drinking and problems associated with drinking. Table 1c presents the results of a chi square analysis of the drinking groups by sex and age. controlling for age, the percentage of males and females in each drinking category did not significantly differ (p.> .05). Table 1d demonstrates that the percentage of drinkers within each category (collapsed over age and sex) is not significantly different from the percentages found by Zucker and Hartford (1983) for 16- to 18-year-olds, with the exception that the females in this sample tend to be heavier drinkers than the females in the Zucker and Hartford sample. In all subsequent analyses, the light-normative and moderate-normative categories will be combined into one normative drinking category. Family Environment, Drinking, and Driving Scale Reliabilities Because the relationship scales are composite scores-~e.g., the sum of four items per scale--the internal consistency values for each scale were computed. The results of this analysis are presented in Table 2, and indicate that cohesion, conflict, culture, religion, organization, and control are measured with adequate reliability; however, the internal consistency values for expressiveness, individuation, achievement orientation and recreation reflect low reliabilities. Thus, the ability of these latter scales to account for a significant amount of the variance among adolescents on the drinking 77 me. A m..ee m cue: .~H~.m u mumsam ecu mexmeemfin. me.xm.eemfie3 .Hee.m u unease ecu .eee.m u mumeem one wees em.em em.em ee.em ee.em ee.e~ m m mm mm as me eoumumammxm we ee.e ee.me em.me em.¢ e~.me e H e on m e ounnmeoz we me.e me.em ee.ee ee.~e ee.em e H es me em he semen we e~.ee ee.e~ ee.e~ ee.m~ ee.em e e no em as ea emcemumna mmHmeom mon: moneom moan: mofimeom moan: me u mom Hoe eceeeouucou om< MOM ocfiaouucou .xom an monouo mcwxceuo ecu mo womaamce phenom ecu on menus 78 emem whee emem 3.2 Eflfizufieh .m em.emue.ee e~.meue.e em.emum.em ee.mmue.em Hugues .a "emcee new» eeueee whomenem sea .HHH eeeem {me $03. emeém €885,135 .m ee.-nm.ee em.meue.e em.emum.em em.mmne.e~ segues .< “$30 Home mated modmeom .HH eeeem £03 Seem Seem bugging .m em.~mue.em ee.eene.ee ee.mmneum~ ee.emue.em umxose .a "Ameeo Hams wanes. mmem: .H ooumuooomenm Bmumerlmfiugoz unmefilgeumfioz mumfimumoe 53m mange see: 8085 ocm noxoom 93 cu :Omeumfimeou «Inbommumo mfixfiuo comm cote: muooflosm mo mommucooumm pa manna. 79 Table 2 Internal Consistency Scores for Scaled variables on Measures of Family Environment, Driving. Drinking and Peer Support Scale Alpha Family'Environment: Cbhesion .64 Expressiveness .38 Conflict .70 Individuaticn .14 Achievement orientation .18 Culture .41 Recreation .27 Religion .60 Organization .52 control .41 Driving: Risky Driving Index .38 Drinking: Problem Drinking Index .69 Quantity/Frequency Index .72 Peer support for: Drinking .30 Driving .40 80 and driving measures is attenuated. Any relationships that emerge on these scales are likely to be very rdbust. Among the drinking and driving dependent measures, the problem drinking index and the quantity/frequency index were measured*with considerable accuracy, while the peer support scales and the risky’ driving index.were measured with moderate reliability. Testing the gypothesized Curvilinear Relationships Hypotheses #1, #4, and #7 specify that families which function on the extremes of cohesion, expressiveness, control, and organization will have adolescent members who are more likely to act out in the areas of drinking and driving. Such a relationship is nonlinear and hence inappropriate for linear regression analysis. Hewever, Cbhen (1978) provides a cogent argument for the ability of linear regression analysis to determine if nonlinear relationships exist. By stepping the linear and curvilinear forms of a variable, in that order, into a hierarchical analysis, one can determine if there is a significant curvilinear relationship after the linear relationship has been accounted for. In the present study, two relationship scales-cohesion and expressiveness-and two maintenance scales-organization and control--are posited to relate in a curvilinear fashion to adolescent drinking and risky driving. In order to test these hypotheses, fOur hierarchical analyses were conducted for each dependent variable, stepping in the linear form of the family environment variables first (Y=bX1 + a) and then the curvilinear form (YébXI + le*X1 + a). The null hypotheses for the partial, semipartial, beta and B weights all test whether there is a unique, significant curvilinear relationship. 81 Table 3 presents the significance tests for the partial correlations of the curvilinear forms of the relationship and the maintenance scales with the drinking and driving measures. Alpha has been set at .1 so that curvilinear relationships are more readily identified. The results of this analysis indicate that significant curvilinear relationships exist for cohesion with three of the dependent measures—quantity/ frequency index (2 < .05) , transition to drinking index (2 < .10), and risky driving index (p_< .01), as well as a significant linear relationship with the risky driving index (L< .001) . Similarly, organization has a significant curvilinear relationship with two of the drinking measures—quantity/ frequency index (p_< .05) and transition to drinking index (25 .10)-and expressiveness has a significant curvilinear relationship with one of the measures—quantity/frequency index (p_< .01) . Finally, control is not significantly related to drinking or driving in its curvilinear form (2.) .05) . Figure 10 further illustrates the curvilinear phenomenon. Although the relationship between cohesion and the risky driving index is presented, the schematic representation of the curvilinear relationship between any of the other family environment variables and the dependent measures would be similar. A significant curvilinear relationship suggests that at both low and high values of variable A, in this case cohesion, the values of variable B--risky driving—remain relatively constant at either a high or a low level. At moderate values of variable A, the value of variable B is in the reverse direction of its value at the extremes of A. Further analyses would be needed to determine whether the actual function is av or a I \ 82 Table 3 Correlations of the Curvilinear Association between Cohesion, Expressiveness, control & Organization and Drinking and Driving After Partialling Out the Linear Associations Partial Prdb. of variables Correlations F Sig. linear R I. Licensed drivers: A. Risky driving index Expressiveness -.03 .26 -- .05 Control .08 1.39 -- -- Organization -.04 .46 -- .01 II. NOrmative and problem drinkers: A. Quantity/frequency index Cohesion -.17 5.13 .05 -- Expressiveness -.19 6.39 .01 -- control .01 .33 -- -- Organization -.15 3.93 .05 -- B. Problem drinking index Cohesion -.06 .73 -- -- Expressiveness -.08 1.01 -- --- Control .07 .74 -- -- Organization -.03 .16 --— -- III. Abstainers and nonrproblem drinkers: A. Transition to drinking index Cohesion -.13 2.91 .09 -- Expressiveness .02 .75 -- -- Control -.00 .75 -- .05 Organization -.13 2.87 .09 -- 83 Risky Driving Status Nbrmative Low High cohesion Figure 10. Illustration of a significant curvilinear relationship between cohesion and risky driving. 84 shape. Thus, as illustrated in Figure 10, a significant curvilinear relationship between cohesion and risky driving indicates that the precise relationship will be either a K. ,I (a) or an inverted m (b) function. In the following analyses, the appropriate form‘of the family environment variables will be selected for each dependent variable. The specified curvilinear form of the significant family environment scales is a U—shaped function. Thus, subjects scoring on the extremes--i.e., scale values of 0 and 4-dwill be put into one group (coded as 1) and subjects scoring in the middle range--i.e., scale values of 1 to 3-dwill be put into a second group (coded as 0). Overall Relationship Between Family Environment and Adolescent Drinking and Risky Driving Hypotheses #1, #4, and #7 require ten separate regression equations for each dependent variable-three drinking variables and one driving variable. The resultant number of regression equations would equal 40. ‘With alpha set at .05, it.wculd be expected that two of these equations would yield a significant outcome strictly by chance. In order to reduce the probability of type I error, it has been suggested that.when the independent and dependent variables are multiple measures of the same or similar constructs--i.e., are significantly intercorrelated-a conservative first step*wculd be a multiple regression analysis, or a canonical analysis, depending on the number of dependent variables. Ruth.such an analysis, one would determine whether any relationship exists between the two sets of variables. If a relationship does exist, the exact nature of the 85 relationship can then be teased out by conducting further analyses on each dependent variable individually (Cohen S. Cohen, 1975) . Table 4 presents the intercorrelations for the ten areas of family functioning. Table 5 presents the intercorrelations of the three measures of drinking and the one measure of risky driving. It should be noted that the drinking measures are based upon different samples. The quantity/frequency and problem drinking indices are based on data from subjects who have made the transition to drinking, while the transition to drinking index is based upon data from abstainers and normative drinkers. Finally, the risky driving index is based upon data from all subjects-licensed drivers. It is evident that the family enviromnent dinensions-r_ela: tionships: cohesion, expressiveness, and conflict; growth: achievement-orientation, individuation, intel lectual-cultural orientation, recreation, and religion; maintenance: organization and control--are significantly intercorrelated (p < .05-.001). Similarly, the two drinking measures--quantity/ frequency and problem drinking indices--the one transition to drinking measure, and the one risky driving measure are significantly intercorrelated (pf .05) . Thus, the first step in analyzing the relationships between family envirorment and adolescent drinking and risky driving-hypotheses #1, #4, and #7—wi11 be a multiple regression. Since this is the first step in an exploratory analysis, alpha will be set at .1 so that overall relationships are more readily identified. The multiple regression equations for the family envirorment scales and the four dependent variables are presented in Tables 6-8. *a 86 Table 4 Intercorrelations of Family Enviromnent Scales Cohesion (1) Cohsq (2) -.09 Express (3) .59 -.15 *** ** Expsq (4) -.49 .30 -.46 *** *** *** * *** * *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** Recreat (8) .67 -.20 .56 -.45 .28 .54 .52 *** *** *** *** *** *** *** Religion (9) .53 -.15 .35 -.33 .25 .47 .38 .54 *** ** *** *** *** *** *** *** Culture (10) .62 -.19 .52 -.39 .20 .49 .45 .64 .51 *** ** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** Orgarliz (11) 074 -021 050 -050 013 060 046 064 .56 059 *** *** *** *** * *** *** *** *** *** Orgsq (12) -028 038 -029 027 -028 -035 -047 -033 -018 -031 -020 *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** ** *** ** COHtIOl (13) .37 -026 .19 -031 052 043 040 .39 038 028 040 -025 *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** *** ** *** F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6 F7 F8 F9 F10 F11 F12 *** (B < .001). ** (p_ < .01 ). * (p < .05 ) 87 Table 5 Intercorrelations of the Drinkingyand Driving Measures Risky driving (D1) Quantity/ (132) .57 frequency *** Problem (D3) .78 .41 drinking *** *** Transition (D4) .08 .40 .38 to drinking -- *** *** D1 D2 D3 D4 *** (E < .001). ** (P. < .01 ). * (E < .05 ). 88 ooo. ”63ch 33:00 :oflumuwcmmuo mag 16 coflmem coaumwuomm "bogged/HUGH ucQ=w>mEU< mmwcgwwmwumxm vmsoo av.m mmN\HH omo. mma. 5mm. scammcou 855% Ema 883353 .m 393.5 mm mm counspé menswm m m mamauflg 833.53, awe/ED mwmam cam mwdmum ungoufiém 32mm 93 mo naming conmmummm EMBED: o OHQMB 89 boa. msmfinoum mmm. awue\:mso m mHQMflum> amoflcocmo th.I vmmxm mmm.| omeoo mmo.u aoamaflmm com. mummuomm mos. 6H>HacH ham. m>waau< 3?: $30 mva. muficmmso moo. Houucoo H mfinmaum> Hmoacocmo mam. m mm.m mm. mm. mo. N omo. ma mm.~m mm. mm. ma. H @353 cofimamuuoo mocmoauaamflm mo mumsvmnaeu m xfies Hmoacocmo msHm>cmmam uwgssz woman—mm: @3qu 05 can mmHmom DCQEOHH>5 338m can now mflmfig Hmoacccmu N. wanna 90 Houucoo whose musuaco 882$ coaumouomm cOwumsca>accH acmem>wflco< Dowfimcoo mmmcm>wwmwumxm 2:. SA 823 m8. «8. 3m. 8:8 853% 8. 83355. 868383 a 2865 an «m 8832 88.8. m m 6333: 833.3, xwocH mcaxcflpo cu cofiuamcmua can can ucwecoufi>cm wwwemm mo wflmxamcm cowmwmummm mamwuasz m manna (n m m 1.5!: 91 The results of the analysis for the risky driving index demonstrate an overall relationship between these two sets of variables (R:.36, ELF .01). Similarly, the transition to drinking index is also significantly related to the set of ten family environment dimensions, although the overall relationship is not as strong (R:.30,Jp_< .1). Table 7 presents the canonical equation for the ten relationship scales and the two measures of adolescent drinking. The results indicate that a significant first.order association‘exists (p_< .05). After the first order relationship is accounted for, there is no significant linear relationship between the two sets of variables (Wilks Lambda, p_> .05). Thus, hypotheses will be analyzed individually for each of the ten family environment areas. After determining scale reliabilities, appropriate scale measurements-i.e., linear versus curvilinear--and the overall relationship between the independent and dependent sets of variables, we can turn our attention to the examination of the specific hypotheses. Hypothesis #1 (Family Environment and Risky Driving) Regression analyses were used to test the hypothesis that family environment is related to adolescent risky driving behaviors. The previous analysis found that the prediction of curvilinearity emerged for cohesion, but not for expressiveness, organization, or control. Thus, in this set of regression analyses, the linear forms of all ten FES scales will be utilized, as well as the curvilinear form«of cohesion. The Pearson product moment correlations in Table 9 indicate that compared to safe drivers, risky drivers perceive their families as Table 9 Correlations between the Family Environment Scales and Risky;Driving Cohesion Cohsq Expressiveness Conflict Achievement Orientation Individuaticn Recreational Orientation Religion Intellectual- Cultural Organization Control *** (P. < .001). ** (B < .01). * (P. < .05). 92 -026 *** -ol4 ** -015 *‘k -002 —.10 -013 -.21 *** -.21 *** —.26 *** -023 *** -012 th ..m we enmmm Or. 93 moderately cohesive (p’< .01); however, there is also a significant linear trend (2 < .01) such that within the moderate range, the riskier drivers perceive their families as less cohesive. Moreover, the risky drivers perceive their families as less involved in four areas of individual growth-religion (p < .001) , individuation (p < .05) , recreation (p < .001) , and intellectual-cultural involvement (2 < .001)--1ess organized UE.< .001), less controlling (p’< .05) and less expressive (p_< .01). Table 10 presents the findings from the analysis of sex as a moderator variable. The literature suggests that the driving profile of males differs from the driving profile of females. If this pattern is true, then the interaction of sex and the family environment scales should account for significant.variance above and beyond the main effects of sex and family relationships. HOwever, the hierarchical regression analysis does not support the hypothesis that sex is a moderator variable. The partial correlations of the interaction variables were not significant at the .05 level. Thus, sex will not be utilized in understanding the association between family environment and adolescent driving in any further analyses. Hypothesis #4 (Family Environment and Drinking) It was hypothesized that families at the extremes of cohesion, expressiveness, organization and control, high in conflict, and IOW'in the five areas of individual growth would have adolescent members who consume more alcohol and.who have more problems associated with their drinking. Among the drinkers, it was found that the predictions of curvilinearity were substantiated for cohesion, expressiveness, and organization on the quantity/frequency index; however, none of the 94 Table 10 Correlations of the Sex x Family Environment Interactions With Driving After the Main Effects Have Been Partialled Out Partial Partial variables correlations F Sig. 1. Licensed Drivers: A. Risky Driving Index Organize -.05 .63 Control .02 .10 Sex * Cohesion .00 .22 -- Sex * Cohsq -.01 .30 -- Sex * Express -.01 .26 -- Sex * Conflict -.01 .46 -- Sex * Achieve -.06 .88 -- Sex * Individ .00 .28 -- Sex * Recreate .02 .14 -—- Sex * Religion .04 .42 -- Sex * Culture -.01 .20 -- * .....— * —_ 95 curvilinear relationships emerged on the problem drinking index. Table 11 presents the matrix of the correlations between the ten family enviroment scales and the two measures of drinking. The results indicate that adolescents who consume more alcohol on a frequent basis perceive their families as moderately cohesive (p < .01) , moderately expressive (p < .01), more conflicted (p < .05), more individuated (p < .01) , and more recreationally oriented (p < .01) . Further, adolescents who report having more problems with their drinking habits perceive their families as less involved in intellectual-cultural activities (p < .05) and less organized (p < .05). The analyses of sex as a moderator variable are presented in Table 12. The interaction of sex and the relationship scales of conflict and control are significant for the problem drinking index (2 < .05) . Specifically, perceived control and conflict are unrelated to drinking problems among males, and positively related to drinking problems among females. Hypothesis #7 (Family Environment and the Transition to Drinking) Of the four hypothesized curvilinear relationships between family environment and drinking status, only cohesion and organization proved to be significant. Thus, in the following regression equations, the curvilinear forms of these two scales and the linear fonts of the other eight scales will be utilized. The product moment regression correlations between family enviroment and the transition to drinking index are presented in Table 13. Compared to adolescents who have made the transition to drinking, abstainers perceive their families as lying on the extremes of cohesion (p < .01) and organization (p < .05), less conflicted (p < .001), less Table 11 Correlations Between the Measures Cohesion Cbhsq Expressiveness EXpsq Conflict Achievement Orientation Individuaticn Recreational Orientation Religion Intellectual- Cultural Organization Organizesq Control *** (p_ < .001). ** 96 Family Environment Scales and the Drinking (E < .01). * .— -008 -.20 -" ** ....- -- -.03 -020 -.. ** -—_ 013 -001 * ..— .09 .07 .19 -.02 ** ...- .17 .04 ** —-— '_011 -009 -001 -013 .... * —- -012 ....— * -011 -.- 005 -002 Quantity/ Problem Frequency Drinking Index Index (2 < .05) . 97 Table 12 correlations of the Sex x Family Environment Interactions With Drinking After the Main Effects Have Been Partialled Out Partial variables Correlations F Sig. I. NOrmative and prdblem.drinkers A. Quantity/Frequency Index * Cohsq —.08 1.00 -- * Expsq .08 1.11 -- * Conflict .07 .79 -- Sex * Achieve -.07 .79 -- Sex * Individ -.06 .53 -- Sex * Recreate -.05 .42 -- Sex * Religion -.02 .65 -- Sex * Culture —.05 .39 -- Sex * Orgsq -.03 .16 -- Sex * Control .02 .51 -- B. Problem Drinking Index Sex * Cohesion .09 1.35 -- Sex * Express .01 .34 -- Sex * Conflict .16 4.58 .034' Sex * Achieve .11 2.06 -- Sex * Individ .14 3.41 -- * Recreate .05 .40 -- * Religion .04 .25 -- * Culture .05 .46 -- * Organize .01 .48 -- * Control .17 5.10 .025 98 Table 13 Correlations Between the Family Environment Scales and the Transition to Drinking Cohsq .13 * Expressiveness .07 Conflict .23 *'k* Achievement .16 Orientation * Individuaticn .11 * Recreational .08 Orientation -- Religion .01 Intellectual- .07 Cultural -- Organizesq -.12 control .16 * Transition to Drinking Index *** (p < .001). ** (p < .01). * (p < .05). H In 99 achievement oriented (p,< .001), less individuated (p_< .05), and less controlling (p < .01). Table 14 presents the results of the analyses of the family environment by sex interactions. Only one scale emerged.as significant QB < .05). Specifically, achievement orientation was unrelated to the drinking status of females (2 > .05) while it was positively associated with the drinking status of males (2 < .05) . Males who have made the transition to normative drinking perceive their families as more achievement oriented. Relative Relationship of Peer and Familnynvironment Influences Hypothesis #2 (Peer versus Family Environment-—Driving) It.was predicted that peer variables would be significantly related to the driving measure, but that an understanding of family environment would account for a significant percent of variance after peer variables had been accounted for. Three items were chosen to measure peer influences: (1) the amount of perceived support adolescents receive from their peers for risk taking while driving, (2) the number of peers who have been involved in traffic accidents, and (3) the number of peers who have received a moving violation. In order to test this hypothesis a mixed hierarChical-stepwise regression equation was computed. The first three peer variables were entered as a set. As predicted, they accounted for a significant amount of variance for the risky driving measure (Rs.31,¢p_< .001). The second step of the analysis was to add the remaining ten family environment scales into the equation. Since there were no prior hypotheses regarding the ordering of the scales, they were entered in a stepwise fashion. If a family environment variable accounted for at 100 Table 14 Correlations of the Sex x FamilyiEnvironment Interactions With Transition to Drinking After the Main Effects Have Been Partialled Out Partial variables Correlations F Sig. I. Abstainers and non-problem drinkers A. Transition to Drinking Index Sex Sex Sex *tfififiifl-fifl'} Cohsq Express Conflict Achieve Individ Recreate Religion Culture Orgsq Control .13 —.00 -.04 -.15 -.03 -.08 -.09 -.07 .11 -.09 2.74 .32 .21 3.95 .15 1.03 1.52 .74 2.10 1.15 o O b (n 101 least .01% of the variance and the total regression equation remained significant, the scale was retained in the equation. The results of this analysis are presented in Table 15. The findings support the general hypothesis that family environment contributes significant understanding to the phenomenon of adolescent risky driving. With the entire sample, three out of the ten scales were significantly related to risky driving. Specifically, risky driving was associated with less cohesiveness within the moderate range and less intel lectual-cultural involvement (R increased from .31 to .44, p < .001). During the stepwise regression part of the analysis, the ten family environment scales were ordered in a way which was determined strictly by mathematical computations. Shach a procedure was appropriate given that there were no hypotheses regarding a theoretical ordering; however,chance factors were nuch more influential in determining the final rank order. Thus, it was decided to conduct a cross validation analysis in order to determine to what degree the ordering is due to chance variations . After the mixed hierarchical-stepwise analysis was performed on all the subjects' data, the sample was randomly divided into two groups and the regression equations were repeated on each group. The results of these analyses appear in the middle and lower portions of Table 15. The regression equations for the two cross validation samples support the conclusion that family environment is significantly related to risky driving, even after peer influences have been accounted for. In both samples, at least five FES scales were significantly related to risky driving. The results from the cross validation analysis also 102 Amosfiuooo oHnfi. ooo. oo.o oHH\o HNH.u ooo. ooo. ooo. on. uoHHucoo o ooo. oo.o HHH\o ooo.: MHo. moo. omo. oom. muomuoom o ooo. Ho.m ~HH\o oH~.: ooo. ooH. ooo. ooo. oH>HocH m ooo. oo.m oHH\o ooo.- oHo. ooH. moo. moo. m>mHaua o ooo. m~.o oHH\m ooo.: oHo. HoH. mHo. ooo. muouHoo o ooo. oH.o mHH\o ooo.: ooo. moH. ooo. ooo. :onmaoo o ooo. mo.o oHH\o oHo. ooo. ooo. HHH. ooo. uoquu Hmooz ooo. ooo. moo. How. uuomosm Hmooz moo. ooo. ooo. moo. uncoooo Home H ”Ho goHuocHHom> ooouo .HH ooo. oo.o oo~\o HoH.u oHo. ooH. ooH. ooo. omaoo o ooo. oo.oH oo~\m ooo.: mHo. HoH. ooH. ooo. mHouHoo o ooo. mo.HH oo~\v Hoo.n moo. ooH. ooH. ooo. :onmapo o ooo. ~o.o oom\o ooo. ooH. ooo. ooo. «Ho. meoHu Hmooz mom. omo. moo. mmm. unmoHoom Home: ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo. uncooom ammo H “mHmeom mHHucm .H .m m N m m .mHm 2890 on 29on 85.6 mm Bygone 988m o deHnHoz mHnoHHms doom Mom coucoooo< comm o>mm mcHHmooz com Duommom Moon Houmm mca>fiuo can Domecoufl>cm mHoemm mo mcoHumHoHuoo may mcacoemxm .mommfimcm COmmmonom omozmoumlHMOmcoumuowm ooxflz < ma ofinma 103 ooo. mv.v vHH\m ooo.: oao. «ma. mmm. mwv. om>mocH o ooo. mm.v mHH\> NHH.I mHo. HmH. own. one. Houucoo m ooo. mm.m mHH\o hmm.| mHo. moH. mHm. mow. ousuHoo v ooo. mn.m haa\m chH.I omo. on. mmH. mvv. m>mfinu< m ooo. mm.v mHH\v vhm.l hmo. MHH. NeH. ohm. omeoo m ooo. mm.m maaxm mom. avo. moo. moo. Ham. umxoau Home: mmH. mmo. eve. cam. ucooooom Hwooz ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo. homoso Howe H "No coflumoflg moouo .HHH m m mm m .ofiw 3890 mo 39on @986 mm coughed onoom m 333:: mHanHSw doom 104 demonstrate the operation of chance factors in determining which scales appear in the equation, and in what order they appear. Thus, one trust be cautious in over-interpreting these results. The only scale which appeared in all three equations was intellectual-cultural involveuent. (he or both forms of cohesion also energed. Thus, it is likely that these two family dimensions have the strongest association with adolescent driving with this sample of adolescents. Beyond this statement, all that can be said with any degree of confidence is that family environrrent adds significant information to the understanding of adolescent risky driving after peer influences have been accounted for. Hypothesis #5 (Peer Versus Family Ehviroment--Drinking) Similar to hypothesis #2 , it was anticipated that (1) peer support for adolescent drinking, and (2) modeling of drinking behaviors would account for a significant amount of the variance associated with adolescent drinking. No precise ordering of the family enviroment scales was predicted. Thus, tre sane mixed hierardlical-stepwise equation was employed, and the sane criterion for retaining a scale was used. Cross validation analyses were again conducted. Tables 16 to 18 present the analyses for the two drinking variables: (1) quantity/frequency index, and (2) problem drinking index. The hypothesis that peer influences would be significantly related to adolescent problem drinking, with family environment variables contributing additional neaningful information was confirmed (R increased from .24 to .39, p_ < .01) . Three family enviroment scales were related to problem drinking with the full sample and the two cross validation samples. Specifically, greater problems associated with drinking were correlated with higher achievement 105 ooo. ooH\o ooo.: oHo. mHH. HmH. ooo. oH>HocH o ooo. ooH\m ooo. ooo. oHH. ooH. moo. muomuooo m Hoo. ooH\m ooH.: Hmo. ooo. oHH. ooo. moHamogo o ooo. HoH\v ooo. mHo. moo. ooo. HHo. m>mHau< o moo. moH\m omH.: moo. ooo. ooo. ooo. muouHoo m ooo. ooH\o ooH. ooo. ooo. omo. ooo. ocHHmooe Homo Hoo. ooo. ooo. Hoo. unomooo Home H .xwocH oconHuoosmHnoHoo ”mHoEoo mHHuom .HH ooo. oo.m ooH\o HoH. oHo. ooH. ooH. ooo. oH>HocH o ooo. m~.o ooH\o oHo.: oHo. mHH. omH. ooo. muouHoo o ooo. ~m.o ooH\o ooH. ooo. ooH. ooH. ooo. moomuomm m Hoo. Ho.o ooH\m ooH.: ooo. ooo. oHH. moo. ooHoHHmo o ooo. oo.o HoH\o ooo.: oHo. ooo. moo. Hoo. omoxm o ooo. oH.o moH\m ooH.: ooo. Hmo. ooo. ooo. vogue o ooo. oo.~ ooH\~ ooH. oHo. oHo. ooo. ooH. ocHHmooe Homo HoH. oHo. oHo. HmH. uponaom Homo H .xoocH mocmoooum\muwucmoov umfloemm muflucm .H .m M mm m .on 2890 on $9on 885 mm cough? 386m o mHoHHHoz 63393 imam Hoo ooucoooua comm m>ommmcHHmooz oco buommom Homo Hanna mcchHHo oco Homecoufl>cm wHoemm mo mcoflumHoHuoo ecu moocflemxm .mmmmHmcm COammonom owfizmoumlamowooumuwmm ooxflz < ma ofinme 106 HHo. oo.o oo\o ooo.: oHo. ooH. oHo. ooo. moHooopo o ooo. oo.o oo\o Hoo. HHo. ooH. ooo. omo. momumxm o ooo. oo.o oo\o ooo. Hoo. ooH. ooH. ooo. moomuumm m ooo. oo.o Ho\m ooo.: ooo. ooH. ooH. ooo. oH>oocH o oHo. Ho.o oo\o ooo. ooo. HoH. ooH. ooo. mowonuo o oHo. mo.o oo\m ooo.- ooo. ooo. oHH. moo. :oHomaoo o oHo. oo.o oo\o ooo. Hoo. ooo. ooo. oHo. oconoos ammo ooo. moo. moo. ooo. uncooso Homo H Axooco mcoxcouo eooooumv "Ho ecouoooom>ommouo .HH mHo. oo.o oo\o ooH.- ooo. moH. ooo. moo. omeoo o HHo. oo.o oo\o ooH. oHo. ooH. oHo. moo. ostau< o ooo. mo.o oo\o ooo. ooo. ooH. ooo. mmo. muouHso o ooo. oo.o oo\o ooo. oHo. ooH. ooH. moo. HoHocoo m ooo. om.m Ho\m ooo.: ooo. ooH. HoH. moo. ommxm o ooo. oo.o oo\o ooo.- moo. ooH. HmH. ooo. :oHoonm o moo. oo.o oo\o Hoo. ooo. ooo. ooH. moo. muomuoom o oHo. oH.H oo\o ooH. oHo. ooo. ooo. moH. ocHHmooe Homo HoH. oHo. oHo. HoH. uncooom Homo H AxoocH oocoowooo\ouoncoooo "Ho :oHuooon> ooono .H o o oo o .oHo 2395 on mHoeHo 866 o o Emoofi “Wazoo o mHmDHoz 280e,. mono How coucoooo< zoom o>om mcoooooz com Duommom noon oopmcm mooeom mo mGOHDmHoHHou on» mcocoeoxm .womemcm QOommoomom omozmoumlHMOHoouoooom ooxoz fl NH oanme 107 oHo. mo.o Ho\o oHo. oHo. oHH. ooH. ooo. m>mHau< o oHo. oo.o oo\o ooo. ooo. oHH. ooH. Hoo. ooHHocoo m ooo. mo.o oo\m moo.u ooo. oHH. ooH. oHo. muopHoo o HHo. oo.o oo\o oHo. Hoo. HoH. HoH. ooo. mommuomo o oHo. oo.o mo\o ooH.: ooo. ooo. ooH. ooo. ooocoogo o ooo. mo.o mo\o mHo. ooo. ooo. ooo. moo. ocHHoooe Homo oHH. oHo. oHo. oHH. uuommso Homo H ixwocH ocHxaHuo ewHoooo. uoo ooHuooHHm> ooooo .HH Hmo. mo.H oo\oH ooH.: ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo. omoxm o ooo. mo.o moo ooo.: HHo. ooH. ooH. ooo. H895 o moo. Ho.o oo\o omo. mHo. ooo. HoH. moo. muomuomm o moo. oo.o Ho\o ooo.- oHo. ooo. ooH. ooo. muouHoo o ooo. oo.o oo\o mHH.- oHo. Hoo. omH. Hoo. Houucoo m ooo. om.o oo\m ooH. ooo. ooo. ooH. ooo. oH>HocH o ooo. oo.o oo\o ooH.: ooo. ooo. HHH. ooo. coooHHmm o ooo. oo.o mo\o ooo.: Hmo. ooo. Hoo. ooo. omnoo o ooo. oo.H oo\o ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo. HoH. ooHHmooe Homo omH. ooo. ooo. omH. uuomoom Homo H Axoooo mocooooHM\>uoucooOo "N* c0oumooom> moouo .H ...m mm mm m .on 2.890 on 39.8 866 oo 88:on 89$ m 333:: 282$ mono How coucooood zoom o>mz mcoooooz coo unommom Hoom Hound mcoxcono ocm ucoecouo>cm mHoeom mo mCOopoHouuoo ooo.mchoeoxm .mommoocm aeommowmom omosmoumIHMOHLUHmuoom ooxoz < ma oHnoB 108 orientation, greater participation in recreational activities, and less family organization. The pattern of relationships between peer/ family influences and alcohol consumption is different from that pattern found with alcohol problems. Alcohol consumption was not significantly related to peer influences. In this case, family environnent influences appear to be nore important than peer influences . Five cannon family environnent scales entered into the full sample regression equation and the two cross validation equations. Increased consumption was significant related to moderate cohesion and expressiveness, less religious orientation, greater participation in recreational activities, and less intellectual-cultural involvenent. The aforenentioned cautions regarding the family environnent scales which appeared in these regression equations, as well as the ordering of the scales, apply to the results in this section. Hypothesis #8 (Peer Versus Family Environrrent--Transition to Drinking) Specific hypotheses regarding the relationship between peer/ family variables and the transition to drinker status were limited to the speculation that peers may play a less significant role in determining adolescent drinking behaviors and attitudes than family enviroment. The lack of more specific hypotheses led to the utilization of analyses which were exploratory in nature. In the subsequent analyses, the sane procedures were fol lowed as outlined in hypotheses #2 and #5 above. The peer variables include: (1) peer support for drinking, and (2) peer modeling of drinking behaviors. 109 Huth.the full sample (Table 19), peer influences account for a significant amount of variance in predicting abstainer/normative drinker status (R:.301,'p.< .01). However, the data in the middle and lower portions of Table 19 demonstrate that in one cross validation equation, peer influences are not significantly related to the dependent variable (Rs.308,'p_> .05) whereas in the other cross validation sample, the relationship is significant (R=.312, p < .05) . The differences in the magnitude of R.between the three samples are small and the associated statistical significance varies as a function of the degrees of freedom—dwhich are larger in the full sample--and random error-dwhich is greater in one cross validation sample than the other. Hence, it can be concluded that peer influences are probably marginally associated with.drinking status. After peer influences have been accounted for, family environment accounted for a significant amount of variance for the full sample and for one of the cross validation samples. Out of the 2 to 3 scales that stepped into the two significant equations, conflict.was the only scale that entered into both equations. It appears that normative drinkers perceive their families as more conflicted than do abstainers. School, Peer, and Parental Correlates In order to gain a more complete picture of the phenomenon.of risky driving during adolescence, three additional areas were examined: school commitment, peer influences, and perceived parental attitudes towards adolescent driving behaviors. The results of a regression analysis of adolescent risky driving, alcohol consumption, problem drinking, and transition to drinking with these three areas appear in Table 20, in the form of Pearson product moment correlations. 110 omo. mo.o moo\o ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo. ouoooso m ooH. oH.o ooH\m. ooo... oHo. omH. ooH. ooo. 88ng o ooo. oo.m moo\o ooo. moo. omo. moo. mom. mmoooxm m ooo. mo.o ooo\m moo. omo. moo. omo. Nmm. aboomcoo N omo. oo.o ooo\~ oom. ooo. ooo. ooo. mom. mcoooooe noon ooH. Hoo. Hoo. ooH. floooso Homo H ”No ecouoooom> moouu .ooo moo. oo.m ooo\m moo. omo. ooo. omo. omo. oumouoom o ooo. oo.o moo\o mom. moo. omo. moo. ~oo. o>oono¢ m moo. no.0 ooo\m omo. omo. omo. ooo. mom. uooomooo m moo. mo.o ooo\~ mmm. moo. moo. moo. oom. mcoooooe noon omo. mmo. mmo. omm. uuoomom noon o uoo acouoooom> moonu .oo omo. oo.m ooo\m ooo.: moo. moo. mmo. omm. :Oomooom o moo. o~.o moo\o omo. ooo. omo. moo. oom. o>oooo< m ooo. mo.m ooo\m omo. ooo. mmo. omo. oom. uooomcoo o ooo. oo.o ooo\m ooo. omo. ooo. omo. oom. ocoooooe Hoom moo. omo. ooo. moo. uncooom ooom o "oooemm oooucm .o m m mm m .oom oomoooo mm 393m omega mm ooowomoa ooooom m oomouoo: oonmooo> mono mom ooucoooom zoom o>mmxmcoooooz ocm uuommom Hoom uoumm xooco mcoxcooo cu economcmoa one ocm ucoecooo>cm mooeom mo oncoumooouou one moocoeoxm .momhomcm COommoomom omozmoumloMOonoomooom ooxoz.< mo oonma lll Hypothesis #3 (Correlates of Adolescent Risky Driving) Adolescents who perceive themselves to be riskier drivers report lower grade point averages (p < .001) , greater absenteeism (p < .01) , and fewer academic expectations (2 < .05) . In relationship to their peers, they see themselves as having more friends who have received noving violations (2 < .001) and/or been involved in traffic accidents (2 < .001) . Further, more of their friends are frequent drinkers (p < .01) . Surprisingly, they do not perceive their friends as encouraging risk-taking while driving, or excessive drinking, to a greater extent than do their safety- conscious counterparts. Finally, riskier drivers believe that their parents are more tolerant of adolescent drinking (2 < .05) and risky driving (2 < .05). Typothesis #6 (Correlates of Adolescent Drinking) The correlations that appear in Table 20 under the quantity/frequency and problem drinking indices are based on the data from subjects who have already made the transition to drinking. The results indicate that heavier drinkers report lower grade point averages (p < .001); however, they do not differ from their lighter drinking counterparts on absenteeism or academic expectations. In terms of their peer group, heavier drinkers perceive their friends to be more supportive of adolescent drinking (2 < .05) and risky driving (2 < .05) . However, in terms of the number of friends who drink, heavier drinkers do not differ from lighter drinkers (p > .05) . The friends of the heavy drinkers appear to exhibit less caution in their driving practices, as is evident from the fact that they have been cited for more moving violations (2 < .05) . Finally, heavier drinkers 112 Table 20 Correlations Between School, Peer, and Parent Measures and the Drinking and Driving Measures Peer support .06 .13 .22 .20 for drinking -- * ** ** Peer support .09 .16 .25 .10 for driving -—- * *** -- Friends with .30 .15 .15 .15 moving violations *** * * * Friends who .24 .10 .09 .11 have accidents *** -- —- -- Friends who .15 .12 .10 .27 drink ** .... _—_ *** Parent approval .11 .14 .26 .18 of drinking * * *** ** Parent approval .13 .17 .24 .08 of driving a- * mu .... Grade point -.32 -.24 -.32 .02 average *** *** *** -—— Absenteeism .17 .06 .31 .03 ** .... *** ..— expectations * -- -- -- Risky Quantity/ Problem Transition Driving Frequency Drinking to Drinking Index Index Index Index *** (p'< .001). ** (p_ < .01). * (p < .05). 113 believe that their parents are more tolerant of adolescent drinking (2 < .05) and risky driving (2 < .05). The results for the problem drinking index are similar to the results for the quantity/ frequency index with the exception that the magnitudes of the correlations are generally greater. Problem drinkers, in comparison to non-problem drinkers, report lower grade point averages (p < .001) and greater absenteeism (p < .001) but no difference in their academic expectations. Thus, they may not have given up all academic pursuits, but their behavior indicates that excellence in this area may be difficult or unrealistic. Similar to heavy drinkers, problem drinkers report receiving more peer (p < .01) and parental (p < .001) support for adolescent drinking and risky driving. Moreover, problem drinkers report lmowing more friends who drink (2 < .05) and who have received moving violations (2 < .05) . These facts might indicate that they either choose or sonehow find themselves involved in a social climate which supports adolescent drinking. Typothesis #9 (Correlates of the Transition to Drinking) The correlations for this neasure are based upon the data from abstainers and non-problem drinkers. The results presented in Table 20 indicate that compared to normative drinkers, abstainers perceive differences in the amount of peer support that they receive for not drinking and in their parents' beliefs regarding drinking, but not in their degree of school commitnent. Specifically, grade point average, absenteeism, and academic expectations were not related to drinking status (p > .05) . However, non-drinking adolescents perceive their friends and parents to be less supportive of adolescent drinking (2 < 114 .01) . Moreover, abstainers report having fewer friends who drink (p < .001) and fewer friends who have been cited for a noving violation (p < .05) . A final point regarding the data presented in Table 20 deserves nention. The magnitude of the correlations between the drinking and driving neasures, and the school , peer and parent neasures differ. Problem drinking has the strongest relationship with the school neasures, followed by problem driving; moreover, problem drinking is also strongly related to the peer and parental support measures of both drinking and driving. Problem drivers appear to have more friends who would also be classified as problem drivers and drinkers. Thus, the teenagers who are having problems with their drinking and/or driving might be thought of as less committed to traditional or adult prescribed rites of passage-such as academic excel lence-and more committed to demonstrating their independence in the areas of drinking and driving. Despite the fact that their behavior in these areas tends to take on an exaggerated, perhaps even antisocial quality, they still perceive their friends and parents as supporting their efforts to assert their adulthood through these two channels. Compared to non-problem drinkers, abstainers receive less support for and have fewer drinking role models. Despite the fact that they do not differ from normative drinkers on academic neasures, they are able to maintain better grade point averages than adolescents who drink more alcohol on a frequent basis. Thus, both abstainers and light, non-problem drinking adolescents appear to be equally conmitted to school; however, as alcohol consumption increases or as problems associated with drinking increases, conmitnent to school decreases. DISCUSSION The findings from this study lend support to the general premise that perceived family environment is related to the transition to drinking and the management of drinking and driving practices among adolescents. However, the findings also suggest some modifications of the specific hypotheses regarding what constitutes a "functional" family system during the adolescent phase of development. It is evident that not all ten aspects of family environment, as measured by the Family Environment Scale, are equally important in their association with the target behaviors, nor do they manifest identical patterns of relationships to the two target areas. In the following chapter, the relationships between family environment and adolescent risky driving, alcohol consumption, problematic drinking, and the tran- sition to drinking will be discussed, integrating the findings from this study with the existing literature in each respective area. Next, the question of how adolescent driving practices are linked to the . transition to drinking and the management of alcohol consumption‘will be addressed by examining the empirical evidence from this study and other researCh and comparing the findings to existing theories. It was the premise of this study that adolescent drinking and driving are linked because of their status as rites of passage in American society. They may also share other commonalities which present theoretician with the task of specifying and integrating the associations between them. 115 116 A third area which deserves mention is the methodological questions and problems which arose during the execution of this study. Finally, directions for future work in the area of family environment correlates of adolescent drinking and risky driving‘will be suggested. Adolescent risky driving. Initially, it was hypothesized that cohesion, expressiveness, organization, and control would be related to risky driving in a curvilinear fashion, such that adolescents who perceived their families as excessively high or IOW'On these qualities would be more likely to engage in risky driving practices. In fact, the curvilinear relationship emerged for only one scale, cohesion, and the form of the relationship was in the opposite direction from what was expected. That is, riskier drivers perceived their families as moderately cohesive. However, the presence of a significant linear relationship suggests that adolescents at or above the median on cohesion report a lower incidence of risk taking during driving, while adolescents below the median present a mixed picture. ‘With less perceived family cohesion, some of these adolescents engage in more risk taking, while at the lowest level of cohesion, many adolescents report few incidents of risk taking. These findings present two questions. First, why did the curvilinear relationship emerge on only one scale? Second, why was the relationship in the opposite direction from what.was predicted? The basis for positing the aforementioned curvilinear relationships rests on the assumption that these four family environment scales are similar to the cohesion—adaptability constructs proposed by Olson and Mccubbin (1983). The stated definitions of the scales appear to be similar; however, their psychometric properties may 117 be different enough to render them separate but related neasurenents of family functioning. The exception to this conclusion appears to be cohesion since it.was curvilinearly related to three out of the four dependent measures. .A second possibility centers around the scope of the circumplex model. Olson and McCUbbin's curvilinear model of family functioning is a broad based model which was designed to relate to other criteria of healthy family functioning. In this study, the model is being applied to individual behaviors which are not inherent qualities of family functioning. Thus, the concept of a balanced family system may not be associated with individual behavior in the same way in which it is associated with other measures of family functioning; the associations are likely to vary in magnitude and direction. Such a pattern emerged with the findings from this study. The issue of the connection.between system—level and individual level variables will be discussed in more detail later. The direction of the association between cohesion and risky driving suggests a modification of the initial hypothesis. One of the main advantages of moderate family cohesion during the adolescent phase of development is that it allows the adolescent member some distance to try out new roles and to engage in behaviors which the family might not approve of. Thus, it was hypothesized that moderate cohesion would be associated with "normal" amounts of experimentation in the area.of driving, and that exaggerated driving practices would be associated with.either high or low levels of cohesion. HOwever, it appears that risk taking, whether normal or exaggerated, is associated with a moderate level of cohesion. Perhaps such an environment is necessary 118 to engage in transitional behaviors, but is not sufficient to account for the differences be’oween normal and exaggerated risk taking while learning to drive. The existence of a significant linear relationship suggests that sone distinction can be made between nornal and risky drivers with regard to their perceived family cohesion. Within the moderate range, adolescents who perceive their families as less cohesive also report more risk taking. Perhaps at very high and low levels of cohesion, they lack the support , encouragenent , and motivation to begin the processes of separation and individuation. On the other hand, at noderate levels they are able to engage actively in these processes although they still require more, rather than less family support in order to manage the tasks successfully. A study by Hotch (1979) lends support for this interpretation. Hotch examined the style of hone-leaving among a group of older adolescents. She found that an agent or active style was associated with moderate levels of family relatedness, whereas a patient or passive style was associated with both high and low levels of relatedness . Moreover, agentic adolescents reported higher career aspirations , anticipation for success, and personal motivation. Thus, Hotch concluded that anong this group, the challenges involved in the transition from a dependent to an independent status were viewed as a challenge which could be mastered, and that the family served as a support structure for the adolescents. However, in families with high or low relatedness, the hone environnent was either too attractive or too lax to instill in the adolescents with the motivation to leave. 119 If sone of the tasks and challenges inherent in this transition include the acquisition and managenent of adult-like behaviors, such as drinking and driving, then Hotch's interpretations fit nicely with the findings from this study. The difference lies in the ability to distinguish between at-risk drinkers and drivers. Along with variations on the dinension of cohesion, several of the other family enviroment scales suggest possible discriminators between at-risk and normative drivers . The hypothesis that exaggerated risk taking would be associated with fewer avenues for individual growth was largely supported. Risky drivers report less of an emphasis on religion, recreation, and intellectual-cultural orientation; moreover, they report feeling less individuated from their families. Perhaps then, as Ronnel (1959) concluded, reckless driving is used as a means to achieve self-worth and adult status. Support for this conclusion cones from the finding that risky drivers report greater school failure, and are thereby isolated from a socially approved form of individual recognition. The literature further suggests that peer pressure and a need to impress one's peers may be prine contributors to the risks that sone adolescents take while learning to drive. Indeed, this study found that risky drivers report having more friends who might be classified as risky drivers. However, even after the effects of peer influence have been accounted for, perceived family enviroment still plays a major role. Although similar findings have been reported in the literature (Harrington, 1971; Sobel & Underhill, 1976), these studies do not give adequate attention to the impact of family influences as they try to explain the phenonenon of adolescent risky driving. 120 Of interest in the present study is the lack of a significant relationship between family conflict and risky driving. The predominant finding in the driving literature which includes family variables is that adolescents report high levels of family anomie and discord. This apparent discrepancy may be explained by the manner in which conflict was neasured in this study. All four questions refer to frequent verbally or physically expressed anger. However, the conflict in the families of at-risk drivers within this sample may be largely nonverbal, since these adolescents perceive their families as less expressive than the families of safer drivers. It may be that the conflict the adolescents experience is expressed thrwgh their driving behaviors or is acted out in sone other way. Alternatively, risk-taking adolescents perceive their parents as more tolerant of adolescent drinking and risky driving perhaps, leading to less conflict over their behavior. A final point regarding the findings on risk taking while driving is the energence of a pattern of what will be terned "family disregard." Riskier drivers report perceiving their families as less organized, less controlling, and less involved in intellectual-cultural activities (a neasure of societal connectedness or conservatism). Within such an unstructured enviroment, it would seem that the standards for appropriate behavior would be difficult to teach and enforce. With the simultaneous presence of friends who are engaged in risky driving practices, it is likely that these adolescents have both the influence and the opportunity to becone antisocial in their driving practices. The antisocial quality of repeat traffic violators has been noted by many writers (Ievonian, 1969; Pelz & Schuman, 1971; Goldstein, 121 1972). Moreover, Beamish and Malfetti (1962) noted the co-occurance of adolescent rebelliousness and a lack of involvement of parents in connunity and societal affairs among a groip of repeat traffic offenders. Since cohesionr-in its linear formr-and intellectual- cultural orientation have the strongest associationS'with.risky driving, it might be concluded that.giving the adolescent a feeling of connectedness and responsibility to others, whether it be family or society, is an important way to promote safer driving practices. Adolescent alcohol consumption. In explaining the findings regarding adolescent drinking practices, it is useful to take a closer look at the differences between the two indices. Prdblem drinking refers to conflicts that the adolescent has experienced with significant others-family, school personnel, peers, and law enforcement authorities-as a direct result of his/her drinking. The only reference to quantity of alcohol consumption is a general measure of frequency of drunkenness. On the other hand, the quantity/frequency index is a pure measure of alcohol consumption, without any ' consideration for the effects that the consumption may have on other aspects of the adolescent's life. Given these two measures, it is possible that two individuals can report equal alcohol consumption, yet experience very different degrees of associated problems. Hence, in the context of defining exaggerated drinking as a form of risk or problem potential, the problemldrinking index is a muCh more pure measure of this concept. The quantity/frequency index may include connotations of prdblems, especially for the heavier drinkers, but the implication is at best tentative; this index can be conservatively thought of as a measure of the range of normative drinking, with the 122 extrene representing a risk potential groip. With this distinction in mind, the results from this study provide sone neaningful information regarding the family environnent correlates of adolescent drinking. The expression of the need to achieve adult status through drinking is most accurately represented as a spectrum of possible outcones ranging from abstinence on one extrene, to problematic drinking on the other. Both extrenes are statistically deviant groups with the majority of the spectrum consisting of adolescents who engage in normative drinking practices. To appreciate the extrenes, one mist understand the normative process. Thus, our discussion will begin with an examination of the findings regarding alcohol consumption. Contrary to expectations , adolescents who consune greater amounts of alcohol more frequently perceive their families as moderately cohesive and expressive . The curvilinear relationship was insignificant for the system maintenance scales of organization and control. However, these results are in the sane direction as the relationships found for the risky driving index. Employing the sane explanation, perhaps the adolescents who are most agentic in expressing their need to individuate are the ones who are motivated to explore actively new adult-like behaviors. Hotch (1979) found this style of hone-leaving to be associated with noderate degrees of family relatedness, a construct which is probably closely related to the cohesion and expressiveness variables in this study. In their quest for independence agentic adolescents are likely to over-indulge during this transitional period. Fron the work of Jessor and Jessor, we might also expect that they will be engaged in other forms of norm-violating behavior as well (1972) . 123 The findings of moderate degrees of expressiveness and greater conflict in families in which adolescents take risks in drinking and driving suggest that these adolescents are actively engaging their families in the process of separation, and that this process involves some degree of turmoil. Olson and McCubbin's results support the conclusion that these characteristics are present in normal, well-functioning families. The adolescents who report limited and infrequent alcohol consumption perceive less conflict in their families and high or low levels of expressiveness. 'Within highly expressive families, the parents may be clearly and forcefully transmitting their values against alcohol resulting in compliance on the part of the adol- escents and less conflict. Sudh a process may also indicate a slower rate of adolescent separation. Within less expressive families, the conflict may be curtailed due to the lack of emphasis on verbally expressing feelings. In either case, teenagers who consume less alcohol perceive their parents and friends as less approving of adolescent alcohol consumption, although the groups do not differ in the number of friends who drink. Further support for the notion that the adolescents who are actively experimenting with alcohol are also more agentic in their style of home-leaving comes from the finding that they perceive themselves as more highly individuated. They view their families to be tolerant of the individuality of each member and flexible enough to allow'personal growth pursuits. Moreover, these adolescents probably have more opportunity to drink since their families encourage recreational activities. 124 Only one area suggests that individuals who are heavier drinkers may be at risk for problems. Specifically, heavier drinkers report lower grade point averages, although their rate of truancies and their academic expectations do not.vary from their lighter drinking counterparts. Since the group of heavier drinkers also contains a larger number of problem drinkers, it may be that the finding of lower grade point average is largely due to the difficulties experienced by this latter group. In comparing the correlations between academic failure and each of the drinking indices, the larger correlation between academic failure and problem drinking (prdblem drinking index = -.32; quantity/frequency index = -.24) suggests that this conclusion has some merit. The group of adolescents above the median on the quantity/frequency index probably contains a fair number of adolescents WhO‘Will experience difficulty‘with.their drinking practices, and the aforementioned family representation provides no specifications for distinguishing between these two groups of drinkers. For such an understanding, we must turn to an examination of the problem drinking index. Adolescents who report problems with their drinking manifest perceived family environnents that are characterized by sone of the qualities of parental disregard. Similar to riskier drivers, problematic drinkers report less organization and less intellectual/cultural orientation. combined with greater peer and parental approval of exaggerated drinking and risky driving, there would appear to be a paucity of internal and external controls against the deviant or antisocial management of alcohol. 125 Viewing an admission of problematic drinking as an index of potential difficulty is further supported by the school experiences of adolescents in this groip. They report the lowest grade point averages and the highest truancy rates. However, their academic expectations do not differ from their nonproblematic drinking counterparts. The future may present sone harsh realities for these adolescents as they realize that their academic expectations will be difficult if not impossible to attain given their current academic performance. The conflict that was predicted to exist in the families of problematic drinkers proved to be a significant discriminator only among females. Moreover, females who report problems with their drinking perceive their families to be more controlling, yet less organized. Within such an environnent, the presence of demands with an accompanying lack of structure might lead to greater ambivalence on the part of the adolescent, and hence, greater conflict. The difference between the sexes might also be related to the donle standards in our society. Independence and careless behavior are tolerated , perhaps V even encouraged—"boys will be boys"--among males to a greater extent than they are among females. The lack of difference in the drinking patterns between females and males—a finding which has been contradicted by other research (Zucker & Hartford, 1980)-conbined with the double standard suggests that females may be more susceptible to criticism for their behavior. Finally, it is of interest that none of the affective qualities of the family environnent proved to differentiate this groip of drinkers from the group of non-problematic drinkers. Nor did four of the personal growth scales energe as significant. Both normative and 126 problematic drinkers appear to perceive their families as possessing environmental qualities which.would encourage separation and individuationr-i.e., cohesiveness, expressiveness, moderate conflict, and an opportunity to engage in individual growth pursuits. HOwever, problematic drinkers report an absence of some essential socializing elements. Perhaps the family‘s greatest contribution in influencing adolescents' drinking practices lies in the areas of transmission of values (regarding all forms of normrviolating behaviors) and organization. The transition to drinking3 An alternative approach to addressing the issue of family environment correlates of adolescent drinking is to examine the perceptions of abstainers and normative drinkers. Jessor and Jessor's (1975) work on the transition from nonrdrinker to drinker status has already been reviewed. For the purposes of this discussion, it is important to keep in mind their conclusion that drinking is a normal developmental phenomenon. Hence, abstainers are a statistically deviant group. According to the theory proposed herein, these adolescents have not begun to engage in the process of separation from family ties. If this is so, then they are likely to differ in their perceptions of their family enviroment. The findings from this study suggest that such differences do exist. Rather than viewing their families as moderately cohesive, a quality which has been associated with normal family functioning during the adolescent phase of development, abstainers perceive their families as possessing either lOW'Or high cohesion. Perhaps the home is either too rewarding or too frustrating to allow'themnto focus their energies on separation issues. 127 Evidence for the hypothesis that abstainers are less involved in the processes of separation and individuation comes from the adolescents' perceptions of several other dimensions of family functioning. First, this group reports that there is less conflict in their families than the conflict which is reported in the families of their normative drinking counterparts. Since a higher level of family conflict is a normal part of this phase of the life cycle, the lack of stress might indicate a tranquility'which precedes the onset of separation stressors. Second, abstainers believe that their families put less of an emphasis on achievement strivings. This finding appears to be largely due to the priority that normative drinking males put on achievement. Here again, the cultural gender biases emerge; by adolescence, males are expected to accept their adult mission.of financial and occupational success, as measured by the FES achievement orientation scale. These pressures are not as strong for females and younger males. In this case, the difference between the males may have more to do with.deve10pmental rather than chronological maturity. Perceived individuation further differentiates between abstainers and normative drinkers, with the normative drinkers reporting higher levels of individuation. Finally, the abstainers perceived their families to be less controlling. In the presence of less conflict and fewer challenges to the central value structure of the family, parents can perhaps afford to be less controlling. The values and models presented to abstaining youth tend to de-emphasize drinking. Both parents and peers are less approving of adolescent drinking; moreover, abstainers find themselves involved in a peer network where abstinence is the norm. The research evidence would 128 suggest that over the next few'years, the majority of this group‘will enter into the normative drinking group. It.would be interesting to note whether those who do not make the transition experience greater difficulty in exiting the family. The negative relationships between drinking and school per- fbrmance, noted with both the quantity/frequency and problemldrinking indices, do not appear to play a part in separating normative and abstaining youth. Both.grcups report good sdhool performance, high connitnent, and high achievenent expectations. Overlap between drinking and driving status. The pattern of findings for the drinking measures are different enough from the pattern for driving measure to call into question the feasibility of viewing the two behavioral areas as outcones of similar family dynamics. Moreover, only 52% of the exaggerated drinkers are risky drivers, although the majority (75%) of abstainers are safe drivers. The correlation.between the two status groups is .25 (p_< .001) indicating that they may share common etiologies; however, the search for explanatory factors should go’beyond an examination of family dynamics. Both the drinking and driving literature indicate that individual and social factors --i.e., antisocial behavior, school failure, and a desire to take risks-ma 'be related to an exaggerated expression of drinking and driving (Zucker, 1971; Zucker, 1975; Jessor & Jessor, 1971; Goldstein, 1971; Harrington, 1971; Levonian, 1969). In this study, risky driving and prdblematic drinking were found to have several similarities. In terms of family environment, a pattern of relative family disregard emerged, resulting in less of a social 129 conscience and fewer internal and extenal restraints. Further, adolescents in these groups perceived greater peer support or modeling fer engaging in these behaviors. Finally, the acquisition of risky behaviors was associated with.greater school isolation and failure. Jessor and Jessor (1985) examined the behaviors associated with this transitional phase in great detail. They found a cluster of behaviors which seemed to be a manifestation of a general antisocial tendency in a group of their adolescents. For example, adolescents who were problem drinkers were also more likely to get into fights with peers, parents and teachers, to be involved in illegal activities, and to have experimented with other narcotics. Although risky driving was not one of the behaviors that they examined, the findings from this study and other research suggest that it may be an expression of antisocial tendencies in a circumscribed group of adolescents. It is clear that the phenomena of adolescent drinking andidriving are complex, involving some degree of overlap. It is likely that an explanation of the overlap would require several constructs, perhaps including the ideas of rites of passage and antisocial behavior. Methodological considerations. Several methodological issues developed during the course of this study. First, it is clear that the kinds of interpretations one can make regarding drinking or driving will vary depending on how these phenomena are measured. At this time, it is not clear how alcohol consumption relates to problem drinking, or, for that matter, how problem drinking is to be defined. Thus, the best measures we have are comprised of multiple items which render themselves amenable to a summation index. Analyzed individually, the 130 results from the separate items are likely to be different and difficult to integrate. . A second methodological consideration revolves around the power of studies to detect relationships between distal factors-such as family functioningb-and criterion variables. This issue appears to be related to the question of level of measurement and has been addressed in some areas of psychology, such as organizational behavior (Schneider, 1983). At the heart of the issue is the determination of how'different levels of a system interact, how'each level can be conceptualized and measured, and how the various levels can be connected to one another. Studies in applied psychology, and in the areas of drinking and driving in particular, tend to show stronger relationships between behavioral predictor and behavioral criterion variables. For example, in this study, peer modeling of moving violations, a behavior, was the best predictor of adolescent risky driving, also a composite of behaviors. The question becomes one of associating group level variables, such as family environment-also referred to as distal variables-dwith individual level variables, such as behavior. The procedures for measuring a construct that is part of a dynamic process present many complex problems. FUrthermore, if a behavior derived from a construct is not directly observable, the error associated with its measurement increases. In conducting family research, it is difficult to obtain significant results because the hypothesized family constructs tend to be dynamic and not directly observable-referred to frequently as distal constructs. Hence it is difficult to operationalize and measure the constructs. The greater 131 measurement error is naturally associated with a general reduction in the power of the study. Beyond the issue of intralevel measurement is the issue of defining connections between links. The associations between interlevel constructs will tend to be weaker than the links between intralevel constructs. Again, the key to increasing the power of the study to identify the interlevel links is to identifying the major connections and to measure them with as much accuracy as is possible. Once a problem within a systems framework has been operationalized, it is necessary to make the generalizations appro- priate to the levels that are being measured. In this study, family environment was defined in terms of the adolescent's perceptions. Hence, the generalizations need to speak to the individual's reconstruction of system level phenomena. In order to obtain a more objective view of the family environment, family interactions would have needed to be coded by an observer. Alternatively, individual perceptions could have been pooled across several family members, yielding a systems-level perception rather than an individual perception. From the aforementioned discussion, it is clear that the kinds of obstacles involved in doing family research are numerous, and the relationships which do emerge can be considered robust-given the general lack of power in these kinds of studies. However, the results from this study should be interpreted and generalized with caution since it is intended to be explanatory in nature and it is based on individual perceptions of a dynamic process involving many interacting 132 constructs. Further studies utilizing sophisticated methodologies are needed. A related methodological issue concerns specific features which affected the power of this study. The internal consistency scores of the scaled items were IOW'tO moderate, reducing the probability of detecting actual relationships. It may be that the internal consistencies are lower than expected because of the nature of the testing situation. These questions were included in the second administration of a series of questionnaires designed to develop and evaluate the effectiveness of a drivers education program. Some students expressed resentment with the length of the questionnaire, as well as concern that their responses would not be kept confidential. These factors probably contributed to the lowered scale reliabilities, as well as other systematic error. The result was a reduction in statistical power. However, an examination of the adjusted R statistics suggests that the shrinkage due to error is not great. Thus, the results that.did appear are probably fairly robust. If the sources of measurement error had been reduced, more associations might have been identified, yielding a more complete and accurate picture of the phenomena addressed in this study. Finally, the ability of this study to detect sex differences was also reduced due to the lowered statistical power and the number of variables compared to the total number of subjects. The psychological literature is replete with studies which report sex differences based upon separate analyses of the data for males and females. SuCh a method is fallacious given that (1) the error terms are not independent, (2) the number of subjects differ for each analysis, 133 yielding different degrees of freedom for the tests of significance, and (3) the value of the error terms vary. If the conclusions made from interpreting the results Obtained by analyzing the data separately fer each sex go beyond genderespecific descriptions to a comparison of the two profiles, the conclusions should be considered suspect. In order to make direct comparisons, an analytic technique which includes a sex by predictor interaction term should be employed. In this study, such an analysis was conducted; however, after the main effects were entered into the regression equations, the degrees of freedom associated with the interaction.terms were probably not great enough to detect the differences which might exist. Since many studies in the area of adolescent drinking and driving have failed to consider these obstacles, the number of sex differences which are reported may be inflated. Future directions. Based upon the results of this study, a number of suggestions can be made regarding future work in the area. First, the overall power of the research can be enhanced by being aware of systems issues in designing and conducting research; utilizing multivariate, correlational data analytic techniques; sampling from large, representative populations; and limiting the number of variables relative to the sample size. Secondly, the theory presented herein considered that aspects of ego functioning may be related to the links between family system characteristics and individual behavior. It may be helpful to treasure ego functioning-i.e., Marcia's four statuses of identity resolution-directly. 134 Third, it would be useful to explore the relationship between normal and deviant expressions of adolescent drinking and driving. An example of such a model has been discussed in the work of Jessor and Jessor (1985). Finally, longitudinal or cross sequential research is needed in order to assess directly the course that different adolescents and their families follow as they struggle with the processes of separation and individuation. Moreover, such research could relate changes in the adolescent's drinking and driving habits, as well as other behaviors considered to be rites of passage, to changes in the family's ability to cepe with the fluctuating needs of their independence-bound adolescents. The suggestion of such a link, at this point, is merely tentative. This study sought to examine the influences of the family social climate on the normal adolescent processes of separation and individuation. It was hypothesized that adolescents' management of alcohol consumption and driving practices, two areas of behavior considered rites of passage in American society, would be influenced by the manner in which they were separating from their families of origin and consolidating their sense of individual identity. Thus, an examination of drinking and driving practices, together with information on their performance and involvement in school (another source of socially approved status for adolescents) would have implications for the amount of turmoil adolescents experience as they attempt to master an important developmental task: establishment of a sense of self that integrates childhood/family experiences and adult role expectations. Furthermore, perceptions of family environment--e.g., relationships, personal growth.options, and system maintenance features-dwere hypothesized to influence these processes by eitherequipping or failing to equip adolescents with.assential ego resources and coping skills. These hypotheses were examined utilizing a questionnaire methodology with 244 high school junior and senior licensed drivers who were involved in a school-based driver's awareness program--Prcmoting 135 136 Responsible Young Drivers through Education. Several variables were examined: (1) family climate—Family Environment Scale (Moos, 1974), (2) quantity/frequency drinking index, (3) problem drinking index, (4) transition to drinking index, (5) risky driving index-e.g., accidents, moving violations, and close calls, (6) school comitment—-e.g., GPA, truancy , academic expectations , and (7) social environment inf lu- ences-e.g. , peer and parental support for and modeling of exaggerated drinking and driving practices. The results were detailed, providing some support for the hypotheses , and suggesting further modifications . Integrating the specific results yielded several important findings. First, family environment variables were indeed related to adolescent drinking and driving practices, even after accounting for peer influences. Developmental 1y, initiation into drinking occurs prior to initiation into driving (Zucker, personal communications). Thus, the fact that more family variables were related to driving practices than to drinking practices suggested that family influences may be most apparent during the period of transition, and then become less obvious as other factors-e.g. , peer influences and societal norms-~increase in salience. A second important finding was the role that family environment played in differentiating "normal experimentation" from exaggerated and problematic involvement. Risky drivers and problematic drinkers perceived their families to be less organized and less connected with the community. Additionally, risky drivers viewed their families as less control ling. This constellation of factors was termed ”familial disregard" and indicated that these youngsters may be ill-equipped to 137 manage appropriately the transitional demands of adolescence. Further, it was speculated that their supply of ego resources may be attenuated by a history of failure to provide adequately for personal needs . Related to the issue of differentiating between normative and exaggerated drinking and driving practices was the finding that social and academic factors changed as one moved up the continuum from abstinence to drinking, or safe driving to risky driving. Specifically, as the evidence for ineffective management of drinking and driving increased, status derived from socially sanctioned activities, such as school, decreased. Moreover, the social environment appeared to change such that deviant behavior was supported by peer and parental attitudes and behavior. These findings were consistent with the literature on the emergence of antisocial behavior, and suggested that family environment may play a role in the deve 10pment of such patterns . Finally, it appeared that certain aspects of family functioning were associated with behavior reflective of normative experimentation and accompanying struggle. In order to begin the process of separation, the family in the adolescent phase of development may need to provide a moderate degree of cohesion. Too much cohesion may make the nest too appealing to leave, while not enoigh cohesion may cause undue separation anxiety. Moderate expressiveness allows the family to transmit important values while also allowing for the expression of disagreement and conflict. Youth in this type of family environment perceived themselves to be more individuated, while other family environment configurations were associated with fewer personal growth 138 options. These patterns were consistent with the findings of Olson and McCubbin (1983) and Koch (1979). Extrapolating from these findings, one can develop a profile of adolescents who may be at risk for experiencing difficulty in consolidating a sense of identity manifest in problematic drinking and/or careless driving. These adolescents are likely to begin drinking at an earlier age, and to be engaged in other forms of antisocial behavior (Zucker & Hartford, 1980; Jessor & Jessor, 1972) . They are likely to perceive the family environment as disengaged, failing to provide adequate affectional ties as well as socialized values. Consequently, the adolescents may turn to peers with similar values and behavior, further consolidating tl'e deviant practices. Sore of these young people may come to feel increasingly cut off from the larger community that could be used as an alternative for obtaining guidance and support. As energy is devoted toward binding anxiety associated with premature or ill-equipped independence, there is less energy and motivation available for mastery of other socially appropriate avenues of adult stat'us--e.g. , academic competency. The theoretical literature on adolescent development provides a framework for these observations. Marcia (1980) describes sote of these adolescents as reflecting role diffusion, though many of them may appear to be at the moratorium stams. Normative drinkers and drivers could also be described as being at the moratorium status. The difference between these two groups is that problematic drinkers and/or risky drivers are probably at much greater risk for failure to make an adequate transition into adulthood, and for further manifestation of deviant behavior. longitudinal data of both a behavioral and emotional 139 nature is needed in order to clarify these issues. Moreover, family life span research, which takes a developmental focus, can further delineate the ways in which family environment influence tlese processes . APIENDICES APPENDIX A STUDENT QUESTIONNAIRE 140 APPENDIX A STUIENT QUESTIONNAIRE This is a questionnaire that deals with a person's attitudes and behaviors towards both driving and drinking. Most people find tress questions both interesting and easy to answer. Do not begin until you are told to do so. For sole questions you will check one or more answers and/or write your answer. Some of the questions might not apply to you; leave them blank. If you have any questions, please raise your hand and someone will help you. THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS COMPIETELY CONFIDENTIAL. No one will know what your answers are, so please be as precise as possible. NAME TODAY ' S DATE PARTICIPATION IN THIS STUDY IS MY VOLUNTARY. YOUR C(MPIEI‘ION OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE CONSTITUTES YOUR ASSENT TO PARTICIPATE. 141 PLEASE ANSWER THE FOLLOWING ABOUT YOURSELF. 1. My age is: A. younger than 13 years B. 13 C. 14 D. 15 E. 16 F. 17 G. 18 H. 19 1. older than 19 years 2. I am: A. Male 8. Female 3. My grade in school: A. 9th 8. 10th C. 11th D. 12th 4. My racial identity is: A. White (not Hispanic) 8. Hispanic C. Black (not Hispanic) 0. Native American E. Asian 5. My parents are divorced or separated: A. Yes 8. No o. My grades in school are mostly: A. A’s 8. 8’5 C. C’s D. D’s E. F’s 7. How many non-classroom activities do you participate in (for example, student council, sports, clubs, church/community groups, etc.)? ______ 8. The number of days I have been absent from school this year is: A. 5 days or less 8. 6 to 10 days C. 11 to 15 days D. 15 to 19 days E. 80 or more days 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 142 I have been involved in an automobile accident (either as a driver, passenger or pedestrian) in which alcohol was a factor. (Circle as many as apply to you) A. I was the driver 8. I was the passenger C. I was the pedestrian If a high school student wanted an alcoholic beverage: A. it would be easy to get a hold of B. it wouldn’t be easy, but if a person really wanted it, they could get it C. it would be nearly impossible to get a hold of I have a: A. drivers license B. learners permit C. neither a drivers license or a permit If you answered "C“ to the last question (#11), skip to question #15. when I want a car to drive somewhere: A. I have my own car B. I do not own my own car, but it is pretty easy to get a hold of one C. it wouldn’t be easy, but if I really wanted a car, I could get a hold of one D. it would be nearly impossible to get a hold of one How many auto accidents have you been involved in: ____ as a driver? as a passenger? as a motorcyclist? How many "near misses" or "close calls" have you been involved in as a driver: A. nearly every day 8. almost once a week C. almost once a month D. only rarely E. never I wear a seat belt: A. always 8. often C. .sometimes D. rarely E. never 143 BELOW IS A SET OF STATEMENTS ABOUT DRINKING AND ABOUT DRIVING AFTER DRINKING. WE ARE INTERESTED IN YOUR PERSONAL REACTION TO EACH STATEMENT. PLEASE INDICATE THE DEGREE TO WHICH YOU PERSONALLY AGREE OR DISAGREE WITH EACH STATEMENT BY CIRCLING THE LETTER IN FRONT OF THE ANSWER WHICH BEST DESCRIBES HOW YOU FEEL ABOUT THE STATEMENT. REMEMBER, THERE ARE NO RIGHT OR WRONG ANSWERS; WE ARE INTERESTED ONLY IN YOUR PERSONAL REACTIONS. 16. 17. 18. 19. 80. 21. I would not be accepted by my friends unless I drank. A.~ Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree When I am with my friends, I feel uncomfortable asking for non-alcoholic beverages at occasions where alcoholic beverages for being served. A. Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree I feel uncomfortable around friends who do not drink if I am drinking. A. Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree At social occasions with my friends, drinking alcoholic beverages is a fairly common activity. A. Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree As far as I know, none of my friends are too concerned about wearing seat belts. A. Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree I would feel uncomfortable asking a friend to slow down if I felt he/she was driving too fast or carelessly. A. Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. DiSagree D. Strongly Disagree 144 88. Driving around, or "cruising“ is an 1.nportant activity among my friends. We do it whenever we can. A. Strongly Agree B. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree 83. I would not be accepted by my friends if I payed attention to all traffic regulations--i.e., if I didn’t speed or roll a stop once in a while. - A. Strongly Agree B. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree 84. I would not be accepted by my friends if I tried to prevent them from driving after drinking too much. A. Strongly Agree 8. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree 25. I would ask a friend to drive me home if I felt I had too much to drink and drive safely. A. Strongly Agree B. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree 86. My friends would understand if I refused to get in the car with a friend I thought had too much to drink and drive safely. A. Strongly Agree B. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree 27. Driving after drinking is fairly accepted among my friends. A. Strongly Agree B. Agree C. Disagree D. Strongly Disagree PLEASE ANSWER THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS ABOUT YOURSELF, YOUR OWN USE AND NON-USE OF ALCOHOL: AND YOUR DRIVING BEHAVIOR. 88. In the past six months, how often would you say you drank any alcoholic beverage? (Circle one) A. Never drank in the past 6 months B. Less than once a month C. About once a month D. Two or three times a month E. About once a week F. Two-six times a week G. At least once a day 89. 3C). 31. 145 How many times would you estimate that you have gotten drunk or pretty high on alcohol during the gas: zgags? A. Once a week or more B. Twice a month C. Once a month 9. 6-10 times during the year E. 4-5 times during the year F. 2-3 times during the year 6. 1 time during the year H. None Over the past year, how many times would you say you gggxg a 9;; after you had had too much to drink? A. Once a week or more B. Twice a month C. Once a month D. 6-10 times during the year E. 4-5 times during the year F. 2-3 times during the year G. 1 time during the year H. None Over the past xgag, how many times were you a passenger in a gag when the driver had had too much to drink? A. Once a week or more B. Twice a month C. Once a month D. 6-10 times during the year E. 4-5 times during the year F. 8-3 times during the year G. 1 time during the year H. None 146 32. During the gag; ygggs how many times have each of the following happened to you? (Mark gag oval for each question or row.) 40 or 8-3 4-5 . 6-9 more BgLs 999s Iimsa Iimss limes Iimss ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) You’ve gotten into trouble with your teachers or principal because of your drinking. ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) You’ve gotten into difficulties of any kind with your friends because of your drinking. ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) You’ve gotten into trouble with the police because of your drinking. ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) You’ve driven when you’ve had a good bit to drink. ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) You’ve been criticized by someone you were dating because of your drinking. ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) You’ve gotten into trouble with your parents because of your drinking. 33. During a 3 hour social occasion, how many total drinks would you have? A. None--I don’t drink B. 1-3 drinks . 4-6 drinks . 7—9 drinks . 10-18 drinks . 13 or more drinks TINUU NOW WE WOULD LIKE TO ASK YOU SOME QUESTIONS ABOUT YOUR FRIENDS' DRINKING AND DRIVING BEHAVIORS. 34. As far as you know, about how many of your friends drink alcohol at least sometimes? A. None of them drink alcohol B. 1-8 C. Several D. Most of them E. All of them 147 [1.) (.0 As far as you know, about how many of your friends have received a traffic ticket for a moving violation? A. None of them have received a ticket B. 1-2 C. Several D. Most of them E. All of them 36. As far as you know, about how many of your friends have been involved in a traffic accident as a ggiggg? A. None of them have been in an accident B. 1-2 C. Several D. Most of them E. All of them 37. As far as you know, about how many of your friends have driven a car after drinking too much? A. None of them drink and drive B. 1-8 C. Several D. Most of them E. All of them NOW WE WOULD LIKE TO ASK YOU SOME QUESTIONS ABOUT YOUR FAMILY’S FEELINGS RELATED TO DRINKING AND DRIVING. 38. How do you think your parents (or your family) feel about people your age drinking? A. Strongly approve B. Approve C. Don’t care one way or the other D. Disapprove E. Strongly disapprove F. I don’t know 39. How do you think your parents (or your family) feel about people your age driving after they have been drinking? A. Strongly approve B. Approve C. Don’t care one way or the other D. Disapprove E. Strongly disapprove F. I don’t know 40. How do you think your parents (or your family) feel about people your age ignoring traffic laws from time to time? A. Strongly approve B. Approve C. Don’t care one way or the other D. Disapprove E. Strongly disapprove F. I don’t know 148 THE NEXT SET OF QUESTIONS ARE STATEMENTS ABOUT FAMILIES. YOU ARE TO DECIDE WHICH or THESE STATEMENTS ARE TRUE OF YOUR FAMILY AND WHICH ARE FALSE. IF YOU THINK THE STATEMENT IS IBQE 95 EQSILX Iggg OF YOUR FAMILY, CIRCLE THE LETTER "T". IF YOU THINK THE STATEMENT IS FALSE 0R -————— --——-.— -—---- --- —————— --- 41. Family members really help and support one another. T F 48. Family members often keep their feelings to themselves. T F 43. We fight a lot in our family. ' T F 44. We don’t do things on our own very often in our family. T F 45. We feel it is important to be the best at whatever we do. T F 46. We often talk about political and social problems. T F 47. We spend most weekends and evenings at home. T F 48. Family members attend church, synagogue, or Sunday School fairly often. T F 49. Activities in our family are pretty carefully planned. T F 50. Family members are rarely ordered around. T F 51. We often seem to be killing time at home. T F 58. We say anything we want to around home. T F 53. Family members rarely become openly angry. T F 54. In our family, we are strongly encouraged to be independent. T F 55. Getting ahead in life is very important to our family. T F 56. We rarely go to lectures, plays or concerts. T F 57. Friends often come over for dinner or to visit. T F 58. We don’t say prayers in our family. T F 59. We are generally very neat and orderly. T F 60. There are very few rules to follow in our family. T F 61. We put a lot of energy into what we do at home. T F 68. It’s hard to "blow off steam“ at home without upsetting somebody. T F 63. Family members sometimes get so angry they throw things. T F 64. We think things out for ourselves in our family. T F 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 78. 73. 74. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 149 How much money a person makes is not very important to us. Learning about new and different things is very important in our family. Nobody in our family is active in sports, Little League, bowling, etc. We often talk about the religious meaning of Christmas, Passover, or other holidays. It’s often hard to find things when you need them in our household. There is one family member who makes most of the decisions. There is a feeling of togetherness in our family. We tell each other about our personal problems. Family members hardly ever lose their tempers. We come and go as we want to in our family. We believe in competition and “may the best man win.“ We are not that interested in cultural activities. We often go to movies, sports, events, camping, etc. We don’t believe in heaven or hell. Being on time is very important in our family. There are set ways of doing things at home. 44-4-4 .4 REFERENCES WW8 Zucker, R. A., and N011, R. Personal commnications, Michigan State University, October, 1984. 150 Accident Facts, 1965-1971 Eds. National Safety counsel, Chicago, Ill. Adelson, J. (1980). Handbook of Adolescent Psychology: JOhn‘Wiley & Sons, NEW YOrk. Balswick, J. 0., & Macrides, C. (1975). Parental stimulus for adolescent rebellion. Adolescence, 19, 253-266. Baumrind, D. (1967). Childcare practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, Z§, 43-88. Beamish, J. J., & Malfetti, J. I" (1962). A.psychologica1 comparison of violator and nonviolator automdbile drivers in 16-19 year age group, Traffic Safety Research Review, §(1), 12-15. Behrens, M., & Goldfarb,‘W; (1958). A study of patterns of interaction of families of schizophrenic children in residential treatment. American JOurnal of Orthgpsychiatryy gg, 300-312. Bell, L. G., & Bell, D. C. (1982). Family climate and the role of the female adolescent: Determinants of adolescent functioning. Family Biddle, B. J., Bank, B. J., & Marlin, M. (1980). Social determinants of adolescent drinking-dwhat they think, what they do, and what I think and do. JOurnal of Studies on Alcohol, 41(3). Blane, H. T., & Chafetz, M. E. (Eds.) (1979). YOuth, Alcohol and Social Policy. NEW York: Plenum. Bdb, S. (1968). An investigation of the relationship between identity status, cognitive style, and stress. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, SUNY at B. Braudht, G. N. (1982). Prdblem drinking among adolescents: a review analysis of psychosocial research. In Alcohol and Health Monographs, No.4: Special Population Issues. Rockville, MD: National Insitute on Alcohol Abuse & Alcoholism. 151 152 Breuer, H. (1973). Ego identity status in late adolescent college males as measured by a group administered incomplete sentence blank and related to inferred stance toward authority. unpublished doctoral dissertation, New‘York University. Brittain, C. V. (1963). Adolescent choices and parent-peer cross-pressure. Sociological Review, gg, 385-391. Cahalan, D. (1970). Problem Drinkers. San Francisco: JOssebeass. cabalan, D., Cisin, I. H., & Crossley, H. M. (1969). American Drinking Practices: A.National Study of Drinking Behavior and_Attitudes. NeW'BrunswiCk, N.J.: Publications Division, Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies. carlson,‘w. L., & Klein, D. (1970). Familial versus institutional socialization of the young traffic offénder. JOurnal of Safety Clay, M. L. (1974, December). Characteristics of high risk drivers, alcoholic and otherwise. Paper presented at the Nbrth American congress on Alcohol and Drug Problems, San Francisco. Cohen, J. (1978). Partialed products are interactions; partialed powers are curve components. Psychological Bulletin, §§, No. 4, 858-866. Cbhen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral science, JOhn Wiley & Sons. Davidson, S. (1978). The level of differentiation of ego functioning in the identity:achieved and identity-foreclosed status. Un- published doctoral dissertation, Boston University. Demone, H. W. (1972). The nonuse and abuse of alcohol by the male adolescent. Proceedings of the Second Annual Alcoholism Conference of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, JUne 1-2. Dickson,'wu, Hess, R., Miyake, N., & Azuma, H. (1979). Referential cxnmunication accuracy between mother and child as predictor of development in the United States and Japan. Child Development, Q9, 53-59. Donovan, D. M & Marlatt, G. A. (1982). Personality subtypes among driving-while-intoxicted offenders: Relationship to drinking behavior and driving risk. JCurnal of consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(2), 241-249. Douglass, R. In (1982). Ybuth, alcohol and traffic accidents. in Alcohol Health Monographs, No. 4: Special Population Issues. Rockville, MD: National Institute on.Alcdhol Abuse and Alcoholism. 153 Donovan, J. E. & Jessor, R. (1978). Adolescent problem drinking: psychosocial correlates in a national sample study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 19(9) , 1506-1524. Douvan, & Adelson, J. (1966). The adolescent experience. New York: Wiley. Divall, E. M. (1957, 1962). Family Develgament. Chicago: Lippinoott. Elder, G. H. (1963) . Parental power legitimation and its effect on the adolescent. Sociometry, 36, 50—65. Erickson, E. H. (1959). Identityand the lifecyle; Psycholgical Issues. New York: International U—niversity Press, I. Erickson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and Society. 2nd ed. New York: W.W. Norton. Erickson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and Crisis. New York, W.W. Norton. Fisher, B. L., Gibbin, P. R., & Hoopes, M. H. (1982). Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 8, 273-282. Fisher, B. L., & Sprenkle, D. H. (1978). Therapists' perceptions of healthy family functioning. International Journal of Family Counselipg, 6, 9-18. Gleuck, S., & Gleuck, E. (1950) . Unraveling juvenile delinquency. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Pracs. Goldstein, L. (1971) . Youthful drivers as a special safety problem. In The young driver: Reckless or unprepared. North Carolina Symposia on Highway Safety, _§_. Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry (GAP). (1970) . Treatment of families in conflict. New York: Science House. Haley, J. (1980). Leaving Home. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence, 2 vols. New York: Appleton. Harrington, D. M. (1971, Septenber) . The youngdriver follow-up study: An evaluation of the role of human factors in the first 4 years of driving. California Department of Motor Vehicles, Rept. No. 30. Hess, R., Szuma, H., Kashiwagi, K., Dickinson, W. Nagano, S., Halloway, S., Mayake, N., & Price, G. (1984). Family influences on school readiness and achievement in Japan and the United States: An overview of a longitudinal survey. In H. Stevens H. Azuma, & K. Hakuta (Eds) , Child Development and Education in Japafl. New York: Freeman Press. 154 Hill, R. (1949). Families under stress. New York: Harper. Hill, R. (1964) . Methodological issues in family development research. Family Process, 3(1), 186-206. Hogan, R. (1973) . Moral conduct and moral character: A psychological perspective. Psychological Bulletin, _7_9_, 217-232. Hotch, D. F. (1979) . Separating fran the family: A study of perceptions of home-leaving in late adolescence. Unpublished doctoral Tissermdon , Michigan State University. Insurance Institute for Highway Safety. (1984, April). Highway loss reduction status report, 1_9_ (7) . Jacob, T. (1975) . Family interaction in disturbed and normal families: A methodological and substantive Review. Psychological Bulletin, 82(1), 33-65. Jessor, R., Graves, T. D, Hanson, P. C., et a1. (1968). Society, personality, and deviant behavior: A study of a triethnic commmity. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1973). Problem drinking in youth: Personality, social and behavioral antecedents and correlates. Proceedings of the Second Annual Alcoholism Conference of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, June, 1972. Washington D.C. DHIEW. Jessor, S. L., & Jessor, R. (1974). Maternal ideology and adolescent problem behavior. Developmental Psychology, IQ, 246-254. Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1975). Adolescent development and the onset of drinking: A longitudinal study. ery Journal of Studies on Alcohol, g, 27-51. Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977) . Problem behavior and psychosocial development . Iondon: Academic Press . Jessor, R., Young, B. H., Young, E. B., & Tesi, G. (1970). Perceived opportunity, alienation and drinking behavior among Italian and American youth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, _1_5_, 215-222. Jones, M. C. (1971) . Personality antecedent and correlates of drinking patterns in women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, §_6_(1), 61-69. Klein, D. (1971) . Adolescent driving as deviant behavior. In The Yourg Driver, Reckless or Ungepared, North Carolina Symposifi on Highway Safety, _5_. 155 Kraus, A. 8., Steele, R., Gent, W., & Thompson, M. (1970, Jure). Pre-driving identification of young drivers with a high risk of accidents. Journal of Safety Research, _2_(2), 55-66. Landis, P. H. (1954) . The ordering and forbidding techniques and teenage adjustment. School & Society, _82, 105-106. Ierner R. M., & Spanier, G. B. (1980) . Adolescent development: A life span perspective. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Devonian, E. (1969, July). Personality characteristics of juvenile driving violators. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 1(1) , 9-16. Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939) . Patterns of aggressive behavior in experimentally created "social climates." Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 271-299. Iowan, C. (1982/1983) . Drinking and driving among youth. Alcohol Health and Research, 1(2) , 41-49. Mahler, C. (1969) . The assessment and evaluation of the cgping styles of two ego identity status group: Moratorium and Foreclosure, two Hentity conflict-arousing stimuli. Unpublished masters thesis, SUNY at B. Marcia, J. E. (1966) . Developtrent and validation of ego identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, §(5) , 551-558. Marcia, J. E. (1967) . Ego identity status: Relationship to change in self-esteem, "general maladjustxrent," and autlnritarianism. Journal of Personality, 15(1) , 119-133. Marcia, J. E., & Friedman, M. L. (1970). Ego identity status in college women. Journal of Personality, £9) , 249-263. Marcia, J. E. (1980). Development of ego-identity status. In Muuss, Adolescent Behavior and Society, Random House. Mattson, D. R. (1974) . Alienation versus expgaration and commitment: Personality and family correlates of adolescent identity statuses. Report from the Project for Youth Research. Copenhagen: Royal Danish School of Educational Studies. McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1960) . Origins of alcoholism, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press . McCord, W., & McCord, J. A. (1962). longitudinal study of the personality of alcoholics. In Pittman, D. J. 5. Snyder, C. R. (Eds), Society, culture and drinking patterns, New York: John Wiley. 156 McKinney, J. P., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Strcnnen, E. (1982). Developmental psychology: The adolescent and my adult, (2nd ed), Dorsey Press. bead, M. (1949). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: Mentor Books. Moos, R. H. (1974). Familyenvironment scale. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologist. Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. (1976). A typology of family social environnents. Family Process, 1_5_, 357-371. muss, R. E. (1980) . Adolesc_e_nt behavior and society: A book of read1_ng' 3, Third edition, NY: Random House. National Transportation Safety Board. (1974, July). Special study: Youth and Traffic Safety Ed. Rept. No. NI‘SB-STS—71-3, Washington, D.C. Neuber, K. A., & Genthner, R. W. (1977). ‘The relationship between ego identity, personal response, and facilitative camunication. Journal of Psychology, _9_5_, 45-49. National Institute on Alcohol and Alcoholism: Traffic Safety Statistics of Young Drivers, 1982. N011, R. B. (1980) . Early assessment of child and family vulnerability in alcoholic families: A proposed nethodology. Unpublished masters thesis, Michigan State University. Noll, R. B. (1983). Young male offspring of alcoholic fathers: Early development and cognitive differences from the MSU Vulnerability Study . Unpublisred doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. Novak, A. L., & VanderVeen, F. (1970). Family concepts and emotional disturbance in the families of disturbed adolescents with normal siblings. Family Process, _9_, 157-171. Offer, D., 8. Offer, J. B. (1976). Fran teenage toyouananhood. New York: Basic Books. Olson, D., & McCubbin, H. (1983). Families: What makes them work? Sage Publication. Orlofsky, J. L, Marcia, J. E., & lesser, I. M. (1973). Ego identity status and the intimacy versus isolation crisis of young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(2) , 211-219. Otto, H. A. (1963). Criteria for assessing family strengths. Family Process, _2_(2), 329-337. 157 Pelz, D. C., & Schuman, S. H. (1971, Jure). Are yang drivers really more dangerais after control ling for exposure and experience? Journal of Safety Research, §(2), 68-79. Pelz, D. C., & Schuman, S. H. (1971, April). Exposure factors in accidents and violations of yang drivers . University of Michigan. Pelz, D. C., & Schuman, S. H. (1971). Motivational factors in crashes and violationg of young drivers. Paper presented at meetings of American Public Health Association, Mineapolis. Perrine, M. W. (1974) . The Spectrum of Drinking Drivers, Wiley Press. Podd, M. H., Marcia, J. E., & Rubin, B. M. (1970). The effects of ego identity and partner perception on a prisoner's dilemna game. Jairnal of Social Psychology, 3, 117-126. Rachel, J. V., Maisto, S. A., Gues, L. L, & Hubbard, R. L. (1982). Alcohol use amorg youth. In J. DeLuca (Eds), Alcohol and health mongraphs, no.1: Alcohol consumption and related problems. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Rappoport, R. (1963). Normal crises, family structure, and mental health. Family Process, 2, 68-79. Ricks, D., & Berry, J. C. (1970). Family and symptan patterns that precede schizophrenia. In Roff, M. s. Ricks, D.F. (Eds), Life History Research in Psychopathology, Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Robins, L. N. (1966) . Deviant Children Grown up, Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Ronmel, R. C. (1959, March). Personality characteristics and attitudes of youthful accident-repeating drivers . Traffic Safety Rosenfeld, R. U. (1972) . The relationship of ego identity to similarity among self, ideal self, and probable occupational role concept among college males. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryiand. Russell, C. S. (1980) . A nethodological study of family cohesion and adaptability. Journal of Marriage and Family Camseling, _6_(4) , 459-470. Schuster, D. (1971) . 'Ihe yourg problem driver. In The Young Driver: Reckless or Unprepared? North Carolina Symposium on Highway Safety, 5. Schvaneveldt, J. (1973) . Mormon adolescents: Likes and dislikes toward parents and hone. Adolescence, _8_, 171-178. 158 Selzer, M. L.,‘Vinokur, A., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). A.psychosocial comparison of drunken drivers and alcoholics. JOurnal of Studies Sobel, R., & Underhill, R. (1976). Family disorganization and teenage auto accidents. JOurnal of Safety Research, g, 8-18. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The integpersonal theory ofgpsychiatry. New York: W.W. Norton. Sundberg, N., Schrma,‘V., Wbdtli, T., & Rohila, P. (1969). Family cohesiveness and autonomy of adolescents in India and the United States. Jburnal of Marriage and the Family, 403-407. vanderVEen, F. (1976). content dimensions of the family concgpt test and their relation to childhood disturbance. Unpublished manuscript. Institute for JUvenile Research, Chicago. VEgega, M. E. (1984). Deterring drinking-driving among youth: Some research needs. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. VErdonik. F., & Sherod, In (1984). An inventory;of longitudinal research on childhood and adolescence, Social Science Research council, New York. WEstley,'W. A., & Epstein, N. B. (1969). Silent.minority: Families of emotionally healthy college students. San Francisco: Jessey-Bass. Wbdarski, J. S., & Hoffman, S. D. (1984). Alcohol education for adolecents. Social Wbrk in Education, 69-92. wynne, L. C., & Cole, R. E. (1982). The Rochester Risk Research Program: A new look at parental diagnoses and family relationships. In H. Stierlin, L.C. Wynne, & M. Wirsching (Eds), Psychosocial intervention in schizophrenia: An international view, Berlin: Springer-VErlag, 25-48. Zucker, R. A., & Riester, A. E. (1968, December) Adolescent social structure and drinking behavior. Personnel and Guidance JOurnal, 304-312. Zucker, R. A. (1976). Parental influences on the drinking patterns of their children. In Greenblatt & SchuCkit (Eds.), Alcdholism Problems in women and Children. Zucker, R. A. (1979). Developmental aspects of drinking throughout the young adult years. In Blane, H. T. & Chafetz, M. E. (Eds.), Ycuth, Alcohol, and Social Poligy, Plenum Press. Zucker, R. A., & Harford, T. C. (1983). National study of the demography of adolescent drinking practices in 1980. Jaurnal of Studies on Alcohol, 44, 974-985. 159 Zucker, R. A. (1984). Youth, alcohol, and driving: A developnental overview. Paper presented at American Psychological Association. Zucker, R. A., & Barron, F. H. (1973). Parental behaviors associated with problem drinking and antisocial behavior amorg adolescent males. Proceedings of the First Annual Conference on the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, June 1971, Washington D.C., DHEW. Zucker, R. A., & Devoe, C. (1975) . Life history characteristics associated with problem drinking and antisocial behavior in adolescent girls: A comparison with male findings. In Wirt, Winokur, & Merrill (Eds.) , Life history research in psychopatmlogyo Zucker, R. A., & Noll, R. B. (1982). Precursors and developnental influences on drinking and alcoholism Etiology from a longitudinal perspective. In J. DeLuca (Eds), Alcohol and Health Monogrgahs, No. 1: Alcohol Consumption and Related Problems. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.