AN ENVESTIGATIGN OF CERTAIN SQClO-LING'UISTIC::_' )- ' ' '. 2.; PHENOMENA m THE VOCABULARY, PRONUNCIATIOND 1 g g. CHILDREN, THEIR PARENTS AND, THEIR TEACHERS-1: i -, IN THE DETROIT PUBUC‘SCHOOLS ; ,:- 2;; Thesis for‘ihe Degree OffEd. D ' D :7 Di: 574',’ . ” MECHlGAN STATE-UNIVERSITY»: r .~ ~ - , . ~ ANNE sums-m HUGHES; _ I , . . ” 1967 ' ' TH 7.35: lllflllM'l t1lil‘flin«\\“lm'\\\M‘Ifiilfifiml *K/ J"; A“ 17L. Michigan State a University 4; This is to certify that the thesis entitled ‘ AN lNVESTIGATION 01.1" (13.11131 SOCIO-LINGUISTIC PHENOMENA IN THE VOCABULARY, PRONUNCIATION AND GRAMMAR OF DISADVANTAGED FREE-SCHOOL CHILDREN,THEIR j PARENTS AND THEIR TEACHERS IN THE DETROH‘ PUBLIC \ SCHOOLS 1 presented by Anne miz abeth Hughes has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M degree in E1 em. Educ 8.175.011 ,6 4/, am My Major professor Date // g 1 0-169 M “N ‘ MA?” iw , @lfmm? £412“ NOV 2 21999 5.? ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF CERTAIN SOCIO—LINGUISTIC PHENOMENA IN THE VOCABULARY, PRONUNCIATION AND GRAMMAR OF DISADVANTAGED PRE-SCHOOL CHILDREN, THEIR PARENTS AND THEIR TEACHERS IN THE DETROIT PUBLIC SCHOOLS by Anne Elizabeth Hughes The purpose of this investigation is to determine cer— tain socio-linguistic phenomena in the vocabulary, pronuncia- tion and grammar of disadvantaged pre-school children, their parents and their teachers and to describe the language behavior of the informants in terms of educational implication. In order to accomplish this end certain disadvantaged pre- schoolers in the Head Start program, their parents and their teachers were selected as speech informants. The speech of these three groups was taped so that certain phonological, gram— matical and vocabulary items could be transcribed and analyzed at a later date. A second interview was conducted with a sample of the teachers who served as informants in the first interview as well. The purpose of the second interview was to determine the teachers' concepts of the language problems of the pre-schoolers and their parents. Their reactions to the speech of the children and their parents, both from their personal classroom experiences and after listening to a tape of the children and their parents, were also recorded so that the implications of the teachers' attitudes could be studied at a later date. Anne Elizabeth Hughes This study used the research techniques of the Linguis— tic Atlas Project and included the biographical data of its informants. The data tabulated were: his age, his sex, his race, his academic experience, his birthplace and the birth— place of his parents, paternal and maternal grandparents, his residences, his travel experiences and his occupation, all of which contributed to the speech information, and pointed to some indices of social stratification in the language of these informants. The study discovered that the teachers' concepts of the language problems of the children were correct in some instances and incorrect and incomplete in others. Some teacher—informants vaguely identified such matters as, the deletion of the final consonant stops /d/ and /t/ correctly, but made no mention of the /d/ and /t/ deletions in the medial position. The substitution of /n/ for /n/ in the phonology of the disadvantaged child was correctly identified, but it was not understood that this substitution takes place at times in middle—class speech as well as in the speech of the lower socio—economic level speaker; this is not always a social class marker. The teachers were disturbed by the use of "seen" rather than ”see" in the grammatical system of the child, but failed to recognize that this may be a phonological item rather than a grammatical one. Though the items in the grammatical system of the disadvantaged speaker were identi- fied by an analysis of the data, the teachers were much more Anne Elizabeth Hughes concerned with vocabulary rather than grammatical usage. Certain grammatical items identified as being those items found in the speech of the disadvantaged pre—schooler and his parent were: The "s" inflection, the double negative, the pronoun redundancy and the "done" perfective auxiliary. In terms of educational implication, the grammatical items identified are of prime importance to the teacher; it must be understood that vocabulary differences may be an impor- tation from another region of the country and is not in and of itself good or bad for that reason. Grammatical usage, on the other hand, can prevent a man from adequately func— tioning in a society which accepts the prestige level speech as that of the educated man. It is important to know that social classes determine language and that speech is determined by the social com— munity of which the individual is a member, not the race or ethnic group in which he is born. Finally, the data of the study reveal naive attitudes toward language and a lack of understanding of the disadvan— taged on the part of the teacher, whose language identifica— tion with disadvantaged speech is hard to analyze due to her guarded use of language in the classroom. It is recommended that education be provided for the pre—service and in-service teachers in the areas of cultural relativity and the nature and use of language. The study concludes with the strong recommendation that the teacher Anne Elizabeth Hughes gain a knowledge of how the English language works so that the instructor can identify the actual language problems and can teach children the use of standard English by the use of "switching devices." All groups should be allowed to retain their cultural heritage with the knowledge and use of the prestige dialect which will help them to be ade— quately functioning individuals in a standard—English speaking community . .AN INVESTIGATION OF CERTAIN SOCIO—LINGUISTIC PHENOMENA IN THEIVOCABULARY, PRONUNCIATION AND GRAMMAR OF DISADVANTAGED PRE—SCHOOL CHILDREN, THEIR PARENTS AND THEIR TEACHERS IN THE DETROIT PUBLIC SCHOOLS By Anne Elizabeth Hughes A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF EDUCATION Department of Elementary Education College of Education 1967 Q t ‘UJ WEHéD h) ,-'- A; 3: a- 5 ; ’3 ‘3 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer wishes to express sincere appreciation to the many to which much is owed: to Dr. Byron H. Van Roekel, for Iris excellent guidance through the doctoral program and for Iris valued friendship; to Dr. Roger W. Shuy, for his wfluxlarly advice,patience and kindness through the writing of tinis dissertation; and to Drs. Jean Lepere and William V. Hicks ikm*their helpful suggestions and professional parti— cipaixion on my committee which made the study a worthwhile experience. Special acknowledgment is given to the many principals, teachers and supervisors of the Detroit Public Schools who served as informants or aides in the preparation of this Paper. In particular, I would like to pay special tribute to Dr..Mary Jean Kluwe, Supervisor, and Dr. Clarence Wachner,‘ Director, Language Education Department, Detroit Public Schools, for their warm friendship and constant professional assistance. Gratitude is expressed to: Mrs. Elizabeth Wright, Principal, Angell School, Detroit Public Schools, for her COnstant prodding and loyal affection which were a constant Source of stimulation to the writer, and to Mr. and Mrs. Charles Lalonde who are always there as this writer's extra ‘ arm and heart. ii Finally, affectionate appreciation is accorded to my dear’sister, Mrs. William Van Hulle, and her warm and wonder— ful family whose support was always with me encouraging my professional career. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENT. LIST OF TABLES. Chapter I. INTRODUCTION Need for the Study Previous Research . . Current Research in Progress. Summary. II. RESEARCH DESIGN First Interviews. Why Project Head Start Was Chosen Availability of the Informants SCOpe of the Field Work Collection of the Linguistic Data Second Interviews . Selection of Informants Availability of the Informants Scope of the Field Work Collection of Linguistic Data Summary. III. BIOGRAPHICAL DATA First Interviews. Population of Informants Sex of Informants . . . . . Race of the Informants. . . . . Age of the Informants Marital Status of Informants. Highest Grade Reached in School. Birthplace of Informants Residents of Informants Travel Experience of Informants. Occupations of Parents and Teachers Second Interviews Population of Sample Sex of Informants Race of Informants Age of Informants Marital Status of Informants. Summary. iv Page ii vii Chapter IV. TEACHERS' CONCEPT OF THE PROBLEM . . . Letter of Introduction to the Principals. Statement to the Teachers. Teachers' Responses to Question 1 Vocabulary Problems of Children. Grammar Problems of Children. Pronunciation Problems of Children. Teachers' Responses to Question 2 Reactions to Parents' Influence on Child's Speech . Reactions to Child and Parent Speech on Tape Teachers Characterize the Speech of the Disadvantaged Summary. V. SOME INDICES OF SOCIAL STRATIFICATION IN DETROIT. The Child's Language Behavior Some Aspects of the Child's Phonological System . . Some Aspects of the Child's Grammar System. Language Data of the Parents. Some Aspects of the Parents' Phonological System . Some Aspects of the Parents' Grammatical System Vocabulary of the Parent Language of the Teacher Summary. . VI. CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS How Pre—School Children in Head Start and Their Parents Differ in Some Aspects of Language Behavior and the Teacher's Concepts of the Problem The Language of the Teacher . . Implications of the Language Study. Suggestions for Implementation of Language Data Findings in Terms of the Classroom Recommendations of this Language Study Pre— service Education of Teachers In—service Education of Teachers 111 113 116 116 116 127 132 133 1A1 1A3 1AA 14A 1A7 148 153 155 160 165 165 166 Chapter BIBLIOGRAPHY APPENDICES APPENDIX APPENDIX APPENDIX APPENDIX APPENDIX APPENDIX .--Map of the City of Detroit Indicat- ing Selected Schools Included in the Sample. . .—-The Eight Pictures IncIuded in the First Interviews to Elicit the Language of the Informants .-—Letter to Principals Whose Teachers were Included Among the Speech Informants .--Statement to Teachers Who Served as Informants for the Second Interviews. .--Map of United States Indicating the Birthplaces of the Majority of Parent- Informants .--Transcript of Child and Parent Speech to Which the Second- Interview Teachers Reacted vi Page 169 17“ 175 177 186 189 191 193 Table \OCDNIChUW 10. 11. 12. LIST OF TABLES Percentage of agreement with adult norms shown by members of 28 families, based on two variables. Geographic spread of the disadvantaged schools with Head Start informants included in the study . . . . Schools included in the language study of pre-schoolers in Project Head Start, their parents and their teachers Schools included in the second interviews of teachers in Prolect Head Start . Population of informants . . . . . . Sex of informants . . . . . Race of the informants. Age range of informants . . . . Academic background of parents and teachers. Birthplace of informants Birthplace of informants' parents and grandparents . . . . . . . Biographical data of teachers in second interviews. . . vii Page 38 39 47 55 56 57 58 59 61 62 69 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Need for the Study This investigation is concerned with some ways in which pre—school children, their parents, and teachers differ in vocabulary, pronunciation, and grammar. There is a growing need for just this type of information about social dialect and language learning. In particular, the children from culturally disadvantaged homes and homes where foreign languages are the usual means of communication present a problem of great concern to teachers. One group of scholars has gone so far as to say: In the deprived home, language usage is more limited. Much communication is through gestures and other non-verbal means. When language is used, it is likely to be terse and not necessarily grammati- cally correct. In any case, it is likely to be restricted in the number of grammatical forms which are utilized. Thus, the deprived child enters school inadequately prepared for the typical lan- guage tasks of the first grade. The greatest handicap seems to be a lack of familiarity with the Speech used by teachers and insufficient practice in attending to prolonged Speech sequences. “— 1Benjamin S. Bloom, Allison Davis, and Robert Hess, 92m2§psatory Education for Cultural Deprivation, based on Working papers contributed by participants in the Research Conference on Education and Cultural Deprivation, June 8—12, 1965, University of Chicago (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1966), pp. 70—71. Whether or not such generalizations are supportable will be part bf the focus of this research. Previous Research In historical perspective, the Linguistic Atlas Project, begun in the early 1930's, is recognized as the most definitive survey of American English usage. Origi- nally under the direction of Hans Kurath, Chairman of the American Council of Learned Societies Committee on American Speech, the Linguistic Atlas of the United States and Canada is important as the most comprehensive extant record of American English usage. Even though World War II inter- rupted the progress of the Atlas, three volumes of maps bound as six and an interpretive handbook were published between 1939 and 1943. In addition to the published works, thousands of field records, as yet unpublished, have been collected. These records contain over 750 items of grammar, pronunciation, and vocabulary, all of which are elicited from topics of ordinary interest and understanding to most Of the informants. These topics were weather, food, health, home, work, religion and superstition, sports and hobbies. Thus reSponses provided comparable data for the inVestigation.2 x 2Raven I. McDavid, The Linguistic Atlas Project (an unpublished paper written in August, 1950, and described as "A brief account of its [the Linguistic Atlas] influence upon research in American English, and its implications forlthe study of the regional aspects of American culture"), p‘ o For future work in American English to be valid-— particularly where questions of regional or social distribution are concerned—-the Atlas materials must be made accessible to the greatest possible number of scholars. It is such scholars that will formulate new historical analyses on the basis of the descriptive evidence presented in the Linguistic Atlas. Such analyses may lead to new interpretations of particular details in the history of the language; or, applied to the study of dialect representations in literature, may throw light on the speech of particular periods or places. One of the most extensive and comprehensive studies of language problems of interest to this research, because of its focus on urban areas, has been reported by William Labov who utilized a sample of the speech of individuals who lived on the Lower East Side of New York City. Labov improved on the highly systematic methods of dialect geography to study regional variation in Speech patterns as proposed by the Linguistic Atlas. This investigation reveals that the first approach to the study of language behavior is a descriptive one. The evidence must include an account of the language of native speakers of the community.“ According to Labov, linguistic data must include: (1) features which are constant in the speech of the infor- mants and features which fluctuate; (2) structural descrip- tiOns which attempt to describe the phonology, the verbal x 3Ibid., p. 1. ”William Labov, "Stages in the Acquisition of Standard English," Social Dialects and Language Learning, NCTE DUblication of 196H Conference, ed. Roger W. Shuy Champaign, Ill.: NCTE, 1966), p. 77. auxiliaries and those areas of greatest variation; (3) a study of the social significance of the isolated forms and systems through the construction of a random sample of the linguistic community; and (A) a consideration of the influence of other languages on the speech of English speaking Americans.5 The conclusions of Labov's study suggests that there are two main solutions to language problems: .1- early training which permits lower class children to enter the acquisition route at a higher point than they normally would (see Table l), 2. special training which increases the normal rate of acquisition of standard English. There seems to be little doubt concerning the needed research in urban areas. Raven I. McDavid, Jr., notes that there is a significant difference between "local standard" Speech in Chicago, derived from western New England and Pennsylvania mainly, and the "non-standard dialect of the blatur ghetto," traced to South Midland and the South. This furtfluer suggests, however, that urban speech study be acCompanied by information concerning the linguistic behavior in tile feeder areas of the urban area under language inves— tigertion. Another aspect of language study which needs furtflner attention is paralanguage and the language of Negroes. StiflLl another problem confronts the linguist and the educabulary served as a beginning for a language arts program for the culturally disadvantaged children in the Detroit E’ublic School's.21 \ 20Dominic R. Thomas, "Oral Language Sentence Structure a11(1Vocabulary of Kindergarten Children Living in Low Socio- E3cial acceptability on.a level where they can.communicate unith others satisfactorily. Finally, it is acknowledged triat teachers will have to be provided with "knowledge that WiLll help them build wholesome and realistic attitudes tcwvard children's language and what needs to be done about it . '13]- 30Ruth Strang and Mary Else Hooker, "First-Grade Chigldren's Language Patterns," Elementary English (Champaign, Ill. : NCTE Publication, January, 1965), pp. 38-41. 31Ruth Strickland, The Language of Elementary School Wren: Its Relationship to the Language of Reading Textfloooks and the Quality of Reading of Selected-Children lCNDmington, Indiana: Indiana University, Bull. 38, No. 4, July) 1962) . 23 If it is to be acknowledged that sub—standard dialects do present problems for children and teachers and that the deprived child, in particular, suffers from the inability to communicate with others on a standardized English level, it must be a problem that teachers should be able to cope with from the child's first encounter with the school experi- ence. As pre-school experience is being provided for youngsters through local and federal assistance, the teacher is charged with the responsibility of providing an approach to the solution to the linguistic problem. Albert Griffith proposes that we are now in a language revolution and that all teachers--e1ementary and secondary alike—~will have to gain professional competence in the area of linguistics as an answer to the problem of adequate (and better) com- munication between child and society. It may seem an im— possible task, to re-educate the teachers of the country who lack this language knowledge. However, if teachers are truly professional in their approach to their jobs of teaching the children the complete and satisfying approach to the science of language, it can be done. They must somehow develop the necessary lin— -guistic competence by:. attending state teacher's college next summer, or by attending an institute here and a workshop there, or perhaps just buy a few books and read a few periodicals.32 32Albert J. Griffith, "Linguistics: A Revolution in Retrospect," Elementary_EngliSh (Champaign, Ill.: NCTE Publication, May, 1966), pp. 504, 540. 24 John Dawkins challenges the words of Griffith, some- what, when he claims that linguistics can be taught in the early grades by any good teacher without reference to further college preparation. The teaching—learning process is one of discovery, which in "the best sense of the term is precisely what linguistics is"! What Dawkins proposes is simple yet solid:33 1 We want to teach a disciplined way of thinking. 2. ... to teach ways of organizing knowledge. 3. ... to teach children to make valid.generaliza— tions. 4 to teach children to ask significant and relevant questions. 5. ... to teach children something about the nature of languages in general and English in particular. 3 6. ... to further the growth of concepts. A thought-provoking question that becomes vital in this linguistic research is the influence of parental language on the language behavior of their children. Doris Noell found that the language usage of parents greatly 35 determines the usage of children. Mildred Templin says, further, that little change takes place after the age of three in the parts of speech used and articulation growth takesplace between the ages of three and four-—when most _‘ 33John Dawkins, "Linguistics in the Elementary Grades," Elementary En lish (Champaign, Ill.: NCTE Publication, November, 1965 , p. 762. 34 35Doris I. Noell, "A Comparative Study of the Relation- ship Between the Quality of the Child's Language Usage and ' the Quality and Types of Languages Used in the Home," Journal %%?Educational Research, Vol. 47 (November, 1953), pp. 161! Ibid., p. 768. 25 children readily learn oral language patterns as they are spoken in thehome,36 and that "aspects Of oral language are very nearly habitual by the time a child enters first grade."37 The U. S. Office Of Education, in January of 1966, granted funds to a project called the Detroit Dialect Study, under the direction Of Dr. Roger W. Shuy. A growing interest in urban areas on the part of both the sociologist and the linguist made the selection of Detroit a natural one. This city of almost three million individuals includes a large number of Negroes, Southern whites and immigrants whose Speech would have a natural effect on the large urban area. The research plan for this study was plotted in three stages: 1. Preparation 2. Fieldwork 3. Analysis38 Shuy listed the aims of the study: As linguists, the researchers are interested in the manifestation of Detroit Speech. As dialectologists we are interested in the relationship of social status and regional background to this linguistic manifestation. We are interested, furthermore, in 36Mildred C. Templin, Certain Language Skills in SEELIdren: Their Development and Interrelationships (Minne— apOlis, Minn.: University of Minnesota Press, 1957). 37Frank B. May, "The Effects of Environment on Oral Lanémiage Development," Elementary_Engdish (Champaign, Ill.: NCTE Publication, October, 19667, p. 595- 38Roger W. Shuy, The Detroit Dialect Study (unpublished progress report, East Lansing, Michigan, April 1, 1966), p. l. 26 the fact that certain patterns of speech lead to conclusions about social stratification, and we are much concerned about how our findings will be useful to the teachers of English in Detroit. We are convinced, for example, that by identifying the features of pronunication, lexicon, grammar and syntax which characterize the less flexible groups in Detroit society, we can help the indi- viduals in these groups to become more linguistically, and thus socially, flexible.3 One of the major concerns of linguists and educators alike is the task of changing the level of Speech from that which is sub—standard to standard English. The first step in this direction is to define the "problem level," approached by the Detroit Dialect Study in this manner: . We feel that any attempt to bring a person from one level of performance to another requires an accurate description of the most significant features of each level. English teachers have made some progress in defining the features of the target level, but they have done relatively little to identify the problem level. Linguists feel that all language, whether used by peOple of high or low status, is systematic and that the most efficient method of teaching involves the teaching of patterns rather than items. Recent developments in almost all the behavioral sciences attests, furthermore, that learning does not progress from items to patterns but rather from one pattern set to another pattern set. In the case of Speakers of non—prestige dialects, this means that one system (systematic though non— prestige speech) is augmented or replaced by“O another system (systematic prestige Speech). The research aims are: to identify the Significant features Of pronuncia— tion, lexicon, grammar and syntax Bf native Detroiters Of several social classes and age. 1 \ 3.9%” pp. 1-2. uOIbid., p. 2. 4 lIbid. 27 Linguistic features will compare: .equivalent responses of Negroes, Jews, Southern Whites, Poles, Canadians, and Mexicans (of dif- ferent social classes and age) who have lived in Detroit for various lengths of time. Geographical and social factors will be recorded because: . . Detroit is Split geographically and sociall , east from west, by Woodward Avenue . . . the investi- gation will further compare the linguistic responses Of east and west side Negroes, Canadians, etc. The »patterning which exists within the various minority groups wfill provide a focus for pedagogical appli- cations. 3 The research design includes Detroit area residents who were interviewed from the following groups: 1. Upper elementary age 35% 2. High school age 20% 3. Parents 35% 4. Grandparents * 10% The selection of persons interviewed was based on the enumerated population of fourth to Sixth grade elementary school children from both private and public schools. The selection was done randomly for the interviews. Older siblings, parents, and grandparents were included in the Sample if they were Detroit residents. The total sample f0? each family included a minimum of one child and one parent and a maximum of these two plus an Older sibling and/ origrandparent.uu x ”21bid. u3Ibid. qubid., p. 3. 28 Fieldwork for the study followed this procedure: 1. Informants were selected. 2. Fieldwork was conducted by 12 linguists and the director. 3. Appointments were set up well in advance of the interview. 4. Each Fieldworker is equipped with a battery Operated tape recorder which is useful for checking accuracy of Speech and later analysis. 5. Fieldwork training preceded the fieldwork. 6. Questionnaire used is designed to elicit several styles of speech and to determine his attitudes concerning the Speech of other Detroiters. The Detroit Dialect Study is of Special interest to this investigator because of some research similarities. The graph on the following page may illustrate these dif— ferences and similarities. Summary Growing research studies indicate the need for more knowledge concerning language behavior. Studies Show that there is particular attention paid to linguistic problems of the culturally disadvantaged child. From the advent of the Linguistic Atlas Prdject to the current research in progress, particularly the Detroit Dialect Study, Specific investigators suggest that an identification, description and study of the phonology, vocabulary and usage of the language is the scientific point of departure. Through ¥ uSIbid. 29 .coooom pafino co oocoSHQCw Hopconmm .: .ACOLOHflco Samson whocomop mo metapflppmv owwswcmq .m megapmom cocoon mo Coauoflgomoo .m one soaosoaeaosooH .H .coflpwsufim COHUNOHCDEEOO Codm Op :UCGE upmsmpw cocoon: mo mfimwc och Show .m mopsumom :Ooodm mo cowpdflsomom .m soars one coaosoaaaocooH .H monB one .LmEEmcw azwoaococm .COHumHOcscosm .cofluodzooo .OLOOoL Ho>mcu .woocOOHmon mo pLoOoL .mpcoama .mEcom xmcc>m .wEouH HwOHquEmsw “HmOonH mHmwQsoocfl wcflpmflmmm 03p .LODOOLHO Locomou ILOmH>LOQ3m oco momflsmcfla o>Ho3p .LOOOOSHU oco mmm3mH>mmBZH neocomoo Sworn .mocopo; Lfloco .cosoaaco oesom poo: mucopmd Ipcmpw .npcopma .Lpsoz .cogpawco hampcoEoHo Load: mBZ :osnao .oooooaom sass ocsnewar oes onSLpem: .DHOLOOQ H osom mcopmsmo a: ”SOSDm SH mHoocom mo Lonesz Hmpoe w "one; Hsoos pHo>omoom Hamsouona BOLLOMOS assoc: escape somanmh nsoooa Haomsa A "poem Hooch assaaaaz mOCOh .q .< .moeHom oaoas cossm mmhom o5>oaaom ma ”Hopoe Haoxgflce ogpwm hoEme coso Hao>noz opooz Caoocflq homecom phospooz xooocmm maoooaoo owhoow :HHxsmam mahom mxsmcpfimm wcomsoo coonEOm Haoodsso homespflonm mHoocom mog< procdflnom maoocom moa< SDHOILoQQH one Op pcoownp< mHoocom coca mpHOISOSQH |o||i .mhocomop Aflocp paw mucopmd aflocp Augmpm omom poemopm SH whoaoocomlopd mo spawn owwswcma on» CH OOOSHOsH mHoocomII.m mqm onoz moxoe oommoccoe oceaogmo cpsom chm>H>wccom muoxmo cosoz mcfiaoswo cppoz xso> 3oz mcmflocH mflocHHHH mmmcoxc< oHco newcoaa Agony as Asamv mm m: “Hanoe neocomoe ocmfiuoom .sa coco mmoa so A&:©.v H oxmmmzzzrzmmmmr—h—t oflcsomflamo meno>azmccom moHLon macfiwcm> ocmazsmz mHOcHHHH ocmfimasoq fissommflz scaaonoo cpsom sesamsa> oooz ocflaogmo cocoz mxospcox OHco mmmcoxs< oommoccoe fldqfimmflmmflz ofiwpooo memoma< Asmov owe flammv :m mma "HopOB muconom mmmmmmHHr—lr-t moflsoam mflcm>azwccom zomsoh 3oz OHco axospcox oflcfimpfl> Hodfiwmfimwflz mHocHHHH ofiwsooo memnma< Aasfiv mm Aaowv ems oma ”Hopoe :oLUHHno noosom posses ooanoso nonpum UOUHCD CHLQHB mumpHOLOOQ o>Hpmz ill manEpOMCH .mocmsaouca mo somaqcuaamtr. if as names 62 chHmsH> so snow 3oz so nasamsH> u a chHwLH> so xto» 3oz 2o xpow 3oz u E meomH< ”pcocmm oommoccoe mu chHmLH> mo oommoccoe u m AmcHzpv oomwoccoe so xto» 3oz so memomH< u z SHOLHOQ "coLoHHco m mem>OHmocoouo mo ocmHom mo mew>onocoomo n m ame>ononoono Eu ocmHom 2o ocmHom n z mcmHocH ”Locomoe SHooH so sHsoH so aHsoH u a aHsoH so sHsoH so aHsoH u z oHosooo ”oceans sHooH so aHsoH so oHosooo n a SHooH 2o SHsoH so oaosooo u z oHosooo noHHeo s oconsm so ocsHosH so comasoaz u m osoHusm so ocoHosH so cstcoHs u z oHosooa “cocoons oon so oaosooo so ssoH u a oon :0 oHosooo so raccoon u z oHoeooo ”sconce oHoeooo so oon no oHosooo u m 7 oHosooo so oHocooo so oHosooo n z oHosooo “oHHso .m mmxoe mo mmxoe mo mmxoe n m mmxoe 20 mwxoB zo mmxoe u z mmxoe ”Locomoe mcmeHSOH so mEQOH< mo memomH< n m mcmHmHSOH 2c mEQOH< Eu osmcmHa u z mEmOmH< ”pcocmm oHosooa so sssosHa so oaosooo u a pHospoo Eu mEmomH< :0 mEmomH< u z OHOLHOQ ”UHHEQ N neocoHa so seoosHa so mssomHa u m oEoooH< zo memowH< zo memomH< u z wHocHHHH ”genomes mflmsooo n m czocx:: czocxc: mepooo u z mesooo upcogmm Loomom czocxc: crosses AoHHeo soonoa nv csocxc: oHosooo noHEo H Amocosmnocmsow Ampcogmnocmsow oOmHocchm m.Locomm Locumm ocm hospoz HmCLoumm Locpmm pew sonooz Hangman: oOdecngm m_pocpoz ucmELomcH a 3oH>poch .mpcocmaocmcm ocm masocod .mucmELomcH so oodecchmnu.HH mqmLoOCH pom unmELomcH Demo on» mH someones: .m use .5 .0» m3OH>uoDcH Lou DCMELOQCH mean on» mH nocomoeu oaosooo oHosooo MEMDMH< mEman< mean< mEmcmH< meman< mEman< ocmHucm accenoo sHsoH SHnoH SHsoH anoH cham Cwmnm SHooH oHosooo chm>H>mCCom «cacao spawns: upmucsm >CMEpou Sunshoo HaaHmmesz HaaHmmHmmH: womcmxn< mmmcmxp< UCMHznmz pedenwz mo EC mo so mo so mu :0 mo 20 mo :0 mo 20 mo :0 mu :0 mo 26 mo :0 mo 20 so. :0 m0 :0 mu so awesome oHosooo oHosooo saosooo mEman< memcoH< mEman< mEman< comasoaz csmHnon aHsoH SHsoH sHsoH oaso :Hmam cham :Hmam :Hmom moocmo :smHnon momcwo momcmo sswacoaz romance: Hadfimemez HaaHmemmHE HmoflmmHmmH: HSQHmmHmmHz capo oaco mo :0 he 20 mo 26 mo 20 mo 20 kc so be :6 mo :0 mo 26 mo 20 mo 20 no :0 mo 20 mo 20 mo 20 pHOLqu pHOLqu pHOmeQ mEmcmH< mEmcwH< memcmH< memcmH< meoan< oaosooo raccoon SHmoH oHso oHosooo oHosooo CHmam :Hmam oaosooo oHcsoeHHso momcwo mowcmo oHosooo oHosooo xpow 3oz uHOhqu HQnHmmHmmHz HQQHmemmHz mmmcmxp< wamcmxp< ocmthoS OHco EFL 5334 Elk: Elfin EFL Sin EFT-4 ER: EEK: EEK-4 2&4 EEK-t Eli-4 ELI. EEK: uaopuom pHOmeD meman< pHOLpoQ oaosooo pHOLuDQ oHosooa oassoeaHno oaosooo oaosooo. oHosooo uHOLuom mmmcmxp< raccoon .o.Q ”D:Ome ”UHHco “pcopmm "UHHLQ "Locomoetm "pcondm ”UHHEO "uconmm "UHHSO "Sachem ”UHHLO "genomes. “unopwm "UHHSO "Accents 64 Of teachers. These eleven units of three informants per unit were selected at random to illustrate the wide dif- ferences and some likenesses in the geographicalesocial environment of-the informants. The remaining data not listed are available for study if they are so desired. ResidentSIof‘Informants This paper has-indicated that the informants differed greatly in their place of birth; this is also true of their number Of residences. The pre-schoolers, 86% of whom were native Detroiters, had, in the main, no other place of residence other than their birth. Much Of this is accounted for, of course, by their youth (4% to 5 years of age)'andi the fact that they were from disadvantaged homes. *This latter fact accounted for their movement around the city from home to home, which was indicated-by many mothers in the interview; but this movement did not extend to other residences outside of Detroit. The parents, on the other hand, only 22% of whom were native Detroiters, indicated they had resided in states other than Michigan. Most of these residences were in the Southeastern states of the United States, specifically: Alabama, in which 35% of the parents once resided; Georgia, in which 20% of the parents had once lived and Mississippi, in which 18% Of the parent—informants once lived. (See Appendix E for a map of the United States and the spotted area from which the greatest majority of parents migrated.) 65 Of the 43 teachers in the sample, 51% were native Detroiters and remained in Detroit during their entire lives, with short residences in other cities when they were in college. The 40% that listed their birthplace as somewhere other than Detroit added, for the most part, that they did not take up residency in Detroit until they were young adults who came to the city seeking a teaching position or traveled there with a husband whose work was in Detroit. This was unlike the parent—informants who indicated that they had been residents of Detroit for many years having moved to the city from other areas when their fathers came up to the North to work in the industrial plants. Travel Experience of Informants In agreement with the paragraphs just preceeding this, it was not too surprising that the pro—schoolers had little or no travel experiences in their young lives. Their parents did travel, but their trips were back to the same regions from which they came as children. The teachers, on the other hand, listed travel experiences which took them all over the United States, including,Hawaii and Alaska, Canada, South America, Europe and the Orient. One teacher mentioned that she had just recently returned from 8 Leave of Absence during which she took a world-cruise which lasted for six months. 66 Occupations Of Parents and Teachers (and/or Their Husbands and Wives) All parents and teachers did not respond to the question concerning the occupations of their husbands or wives, or even their own in the case of the working—wife parent, but a significant enough number of replies makes these data of interest to the social implication of this speech study. Among the answers given by parents, the following occupations were listed: Parents' Occupations Factory Worker 10 Grocery Cashier Plant Protection Press Operator 2 Key Punch Operator Secretary Grocery Clerk Dental Technician Grinder Truck Driver 11 Welder Gas Heat Worker Solderer Construction Worker 6 Machine Operator 3 Painter Auto Wash Owner Auto Wash Worker 4 Parking Lot Attendant Tool Company Employee Railroad Switchman Bartender Drug Company Employee Caterer Employee Electrical Worker City Employee Auto Worker Bus Driver 2 Are Welder 3 Boiler Operator 2 Laborer 2 Post Office Clerk 5 Michigan Gas Company Machinist 2 Stamp Die Man Musician Mechanic 5 Forger Operator Hammer Man Chrysler Inspector Insurance Agent Social Security Office Steel Maintenance Worker Car Lott Manager Boat Oiler U.S. Navy Asst. Principal Unemployed 7 Metal Pourer Steel Worker Beef-Boner Factory Foreman Ford Motor Company Worker Student Heat Treater Pipe Fitter Bump and Paint Man Medical Retiree 67 The following occupations were listed by the teachers as those jobs held by their husbands or wives, as the case may be. Occupations of Teachers' Husbands or Wives Teacher 3 Auto Executive Crysler Accountant Jam Handy Writer (Advertising) Pharmacy Owner Press Operator Physical Therapist Cleaner and Presser Orchestra Leader Exports Executive Second Interviews The second set of interviews included only one set of informants, the teachers included in the first interviews. As stated in Chapter II, thirty of the original forty-three teachers were selected by random digits using a table of randomly ordered digits. The population of the second interviews was quite different from the original informants and the information sought more the teacher's concept Of her job than simply data concerned with language behavior of the three sets of informants. Population of Sample The teachers who were interviewed for the second time in this study of Speech behavior were 30 in number or 70% of the teachers interviewed initially. 68 Sex of Informants In the initial group of teachers interviewed, 41 of the informants were female and two were male. In the second set of interviews including 30 teachers, 29 teachers were female and one was male. Race of Informants In the initial group of teachers interviewed, 22 were Negro and 21 were CaUcasian. In this second set of inter- , views including 30 teachers, 20 were Negro and 10 were white. Age of Informants The teachers in the second interviews were smaller in total number but exhibited a very wide range in age dif- ferences. The youngest teacher in the sample was 24 years of age and the chronologically oldest teacher was 58 years old. Marital Status of Informants Unlike the first sample which included within its number 37% of the teachers who were married and 63% of whom were single, this second interview included an exact 50% of the teachers who were single and 50% of whom were married, including one teacher who had been widowed. At this point it is Of interest to see a comparable biographical chart of the teachers included in the second interview. Each teacher is given an interview number (see Table 12). TABLE l2.-—Biographical data of teachers in second interviews. 69 Interview Number Race Age Degre e Held Birthplace Other Residence Birthplaces M:F; M-GMch P-GM:GF Occupation (husband-Wife 10‘ Male Informant ll l2 l3 l4 15 57 48 \J" U2 58 44 4O 24 53 M. B.S. COT > 2:11 A I in Ed.+ M. Work Q) ..r g-o .Ed. Ohio Detroit Indiana W.Virginia Detroit S.Carolina Penna. Lefroit Illinois Detroit B.W.Ind1es N.Carolina' Detroit Detroit Detroit Ohio (24 yrs.) Detroit (33 Yrs.) None Ind. (11 yrs.) Detroit (23 yrs.) W. Va. (5 yrs.) Detroit (24 yrs.) Detroit (1-18) Georgia (18—23) N.Y.C. (23-35) Caiir.(35-41) Detroit (41—48 years) S.C. (4 yrs.) Detroit (48 yrs. V Penna. (1 yr.) Illinois (2 yrs. Detroit (33 yrs.) v Illincis (41 yrs.) Detroit (12 yrs.) None B.W.I.(ll yrs.) Detroit (47 yrs.) N.C. (20 yrs.) Detroit (24 yrs. v v Detroit (20 yrs. N.C. (8 yrs.) Detroit (12 yrs. v Detroit (1 yr.) Ohio (20 yrs.) Detroit (3 yrs-) None M:Oh10 F:Ohio MGM: England MGF: England POM: Penna. PGF: Penna. MzDetroit F:Penna. MGM: Italy MGF: Detroit PGM: Ireland PGF: Ireland MzPoland F:Czechoslovakia MGMzPoland PGM:Czech. PGF:Czech. MzTennessee F:V1rginia MGM:Tennessee MGFzTennessee PGM:Virgin1a PGF2Virginia M:Ireland F:V1rginia MGlereland MGlereland PGM:Virginia PGF:Virginia 4:8. Carolina F:S. Carolina MGM:S.Carolina MGF:S.Carolina PGM:S.Carolina PGF:S.Carolina M:Alabama FzPenna. MGM:Georgla MGcheorgia POMzFlorida PGcmeorgia MzGeorgia F:Georgia MGM:Georgia FCP:Georg1a PGM:Georgia PGcmeorgia M:Illinois :Tennessee MGlellinOis MGF:IllinOis PGM:Tennessee PGFzTennessee M:New York szetroit MGM:New York MGF:Ireland POMzcermany PGF:Germany M:Jamaica F:Cuba MGMzJamaica MGF:Jamaica PGM:Cuba PGF:Cuba MzN. Carolina FzN. Carolina MGM:N. Carolina MGFzN. Carolina PGM:N.Carolina PGF:N.Carolina Mzs. Carolina F:S. Carolina MGM:S. Carolina MGFzs. Carolina PGM:S.Carolina PGF:S.Carolina M:Ohio F2Alabama MGMzPenna. MGsz. Virginia PGMzAlabama PGF:V1rginia Mzcermany FzDetroit MGM:Germany MGFzGermany PGM:NOt known PGFzNot known Widow Single Single Teacher Auto Executive Owns a Pharmacy Accountant TLBCLCL' Wife in a Homemaker Exports BUSIUcSE Bartender Single Single Lawyer TABLE 12.—-Continued Interview Number 16 17 18 19 2O 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 Race Age Degree Held U7 M.Ed. .S.in Ed.+ B.Ed. 7O Birthplace Detroit Detroit Detroit Alabama Detroit Alabama New York Tennessee Detroit Detroit Detroit N.Dakota Detroit Detroit Detroit Other Residence None None None Alabama (10 yrs.) Detroit (21 yrs.) None Alabama (26 yrs. Detroit (10 yrs. New York(14 yrs. Detroit (33 yrs. ~./ Tennessee(2 yrs. Missouri (4 yrs. Ohio (15 yrs.) Detroit (8 yrs.) \./ None None N.Dakota(l4 yrs.) Detroit (23 yrs.) None None None Birthplaces MzF; M—GM:GF P-GMch MzDetroit FzRumania MGM:Detroit MGFzDetroit PGM:Rumania PGFzRumania MzTennessee F:Tennessee MGMzTennessee MGFzTennessee PGM:Tennessee PGF:Tennessee M:Georgia F:Mississippi MGMzGeorgia MGcmeorgia PGM:Mississippi PGF:Mississippi M:Alabama FzAlabama MGMzAlabama MGFzAlabama PGM1Alabama PGFzAlabama M:Georgia F:Virginia MGM:Ireland MGF:Ireland PGM:Virginia PGF:Virginia MzAlabama F:Alabama MGM:Alabama MGPzAlabama PGM:Alabama PGFzAlabama M:New York letaly MGM:Italy MGletaly PGM:Italy FGletaly M:Mississippi FzTennessee MGM:Tennessee NGFzMississippi PGM:Mississippi PGF:Mississippi MzMichigan FzDetroit MGM:Nichigan MGlereland PGM:England PGF:Germany M:Canada FzMissouri MGM:Canada MGF:Canada PGM:Canada PGF:Canada MzAlabama F:Arkansas MGMzFlorida MGF:Florida PGMzArkansas PGF:Arkansas Mzs. Dakota F:Massachusetts MGMzGermany MGF:Germany PGM:Canada PGF:Canada MzMichigan FzAlabama MGM:Canada MGF:Canada PGM:Alabama PGFzAlabama M:Michigan F:Spain MGM:Nassachusetts MGF:Massachusetts PGM:Spain PGF:Spain M:Mississippi FzMississippi MGM:Nississippi MGF:Mississippi PGM:Mississippi PGFzMississippi Occupation (husband-Wifd Single Auto Plant Office Employee Single Government Employee Single Writer Cleaner and Presser Single Single Single Single Single Librarian Single 71 Summary As was stated briefly in this chapter, the systematic method of using dialect geography was used to study differ- ences in language patterns. The differences were many as can be studied in Tables 6, 7, 8, 9, 10 and 11 which were con- cerned with informants included in the first interviews of this study. The data reported in Table 12 were reports of the biographical data of informants of the second interviews. Due to the fact that this was a study of the language behavior of the pre-school child, his teacher and his parent, the sample included a very large percentage of females among the adults, i.e., 95 per cent of the total group of both the parents and the teachers. This was probably a most natural sample, however, for the parent most likely to accompany a pre-schooler to the school would be the mother, for the father, as the financial partner in most marriages, would be at his daily work. The teacher of the pre—schooler, too, would most likely be a woman as the greater majority of primary teachers are women. In a comparative study of speech behavior it would be probably most natural to use the language of the mother and the female teacher for they are around the young child more often than is the male teacher or even the father, who may not reside in the home. The race of the informants was heavily Negro within the children and parents' group, i.e., 141 Negro children and 15 Caucasian children; 140 Negro parents and 15 Caucasian 72 parents. This was probably due to the fact that the larger percentage of children in the Head Start Project in the Detroit Public Schools were Negro. The teachers were more evenly divided, i.e., 22 Negro teachers and 21 Caucasian teachers. The age of the informants was very similar for the children for they all had to be pre—kindergarten age to be accepted in the Proaect. The parents were younger in age, as an average, than were the teachers. Between the ages of 41 to 50 years and above we found 39% of the teachers; only 12% of the parents fell in this same chronological bracket. There was a wide difference of academic background reported among the parents and teachers who served as infor- mants in this study. Seventy—two per cent of the parents reported that they had not finished high school; only 4% of the parents had any college experience at all, with no parents stating that they held a college degree. Naturally, on the other hand, all of the teachers stated that they held at least a bachelor's degree, with 65% of the teachers reporting that they had work beyond the four year bachelor's degree and 12% said they held a master's degree. The children had done very little traveling outside of their home city of Detroit and their parents said they traveled between their native states, the majority of which were in the Southeastern United States, and their adopted city of Detroit. In contrast, the teachers reported having 73 ‘traveled extensively in the United States and many reported having traveled to Europe and other parts of the world. The pre—schoolers, too, held the highest per cent rate, among the informants, as native Detroiters, i.e., the children were 86% native Detroiters; the parents were 22% native Detroiters, and the teachers were 51% native Detroiters. In terms of the birthplaces of parents and grand- parents, we find the oldest generation reported were more likely to be born in other countries, i.e., England, Italy, Czechoslovakia, Germany, Poland, Hungary, Ireland, Spain, and Canada. The parents and grandparents of the pre— schoolers' parents tended to be natives of the Southeastern region of our country. Finally, the occupations reported by the parents and teachers differed. More occupations in the unskilled and semi-skilled classes were reported by the parents; occupa- tions which are recognized as professions and businesses, or "white collar” kinds of work were reported by the teachers. In conclusion, the biographical data of teachers included in the second interviews is presented in chart form indicating that which was previously reported as part of the data of the first interview as the teachers were the same in both parts of the study; the number is simply smaller. 74 \ Chapter IV presents an analysis of the teachers' concept of the language problem from their own observations and experiences with the pre—school child and his parent from disadvantaged areas in the city. CHAPTER IV TEACHERS' CONCEPT OF THE PROBLEM The real worth of language knowledge in terms of curriculum development and revision depends greatly on the teachers' concept of the problem. With the simple recognition of the need to change, the teacher will normally seek data which describe the problem and propose possible solutions to it. The previous chapter described some of the speech characteristics of the pre-school child, his parent and his teacher. The second interviews in this study were directed solely to the teachers of the pre—schoolers in Project Head Start in an attempt to determine the teachers' concept of the language problems of her class. Letter of Introduction to the Principals To introduce the study to the principals whose teachers would take part in the second set of interviews, a.letter was sent to the schools asking for permission to Speak to the teachers who had participated in the Project. This letter stated that the study described and identified the speech of children and adults in Detroit for possible future in—service training and curriculum revision for teachers. It went on to say that the teachers would be 75 76 asked to react to questions concerned with their opinions of Detroit speech. The questions were then stated for the principals' information. (See Appendix D for a c0py of the letter sent to the principals.) Statement to the Teachers Enclosed with the letter sent to the principals was a statement to the teachers which asked them to react to specific questions at the time of interview. (See Appendix E for a copy of the statement to the teachers.) The first two questions in the statement were pro- posed to the teachers in an attempt to get the teachers to state their opinions concerning the language problems of children. These questions were: 1. What do you think are the major problems your children have with vocabulary, grammar and pronunciation? 2. In what ways does the language of the parents influence children in your class? )What problems with vocabulary, grammar and pronunciation in the language of the parent are reflected in the problems of the child? The teacher was then asked to listen and react to a tape of the Speech of children who were pre-schoolers in the Head Start program. The final five questions were proposed to obtain further language data from the teachers rather than to 77 determine her concept of the problem. For that reason the data gathered from the last five questions were included in the evidence discussed in Chapter V on language data. Teachers' Responses to Question 1 For purposes of anonymity, the thirty teachers in the study were given numbers 1 to 30 and will be referred to by these numbers throughout this chapter. Vocabulary Problems of Children: Comments Most Often Used Teachers' reaction to the question: What do you think are the major problems your children have with vocabulary . . . ? Teacher Interview Vocabulary # . 1 Their vocabulary is very small-—for most of them. 2 Some had a vocabulary of about a hundred and some words, I'd say; no more than that. They got along fine with what they knew. They didn't have any trouble expressing themselves. They knew the important words for them to get along okay. Some could talk your foot off. I mean, they just knew everything. The quieter ones were the ones who didn't have a large vocabulary. The ones who were always talking had words that you wouldn't imagine a child that age having. They just knew a lot. 3 Generally, their vocabulary was very limited, but then they were very young, pre—school children. Their trouble was the use of dialect for they said hal_for now} It was southern dialect among some of the children which caused them to use the wrong words. 4 In some cases I can't understand the children at all. Of course, sometimes the children won't talk at all, or else they speak in very low tones. The vocabulary is very limited. When we had free Teacher Interview 78 Vocabulary discussions, I like to draw out the children and ask them questions. They would answer with one word, for instance; I suppose this is normal enough. In the Head Start program, the children come with a very meager vocabulary, I would say. I think it's because of the background of the home and the lack of books at home, the lack of communication with the family, especially, if there are only one or two children in the family. Perhaps if there are more children in the family communication might be a bit better. They might have a few more words in their vocabulary. They have a lack of vocabulary simply for the reason that they have not been encouraged to talk; they may just gesture for a doll. They would say, "Mrs. , I want that, you see, that there thing,” So I feel it's a lack of speech, a lack of talking in the home, perhaps, or, maybe I've commented about this because I like to talk. We have definite problems with vocabulary because the children haven't been exposed to many words. Just simple things that they just don't seem to understand. They have a definite lack of vocabulary. I find that with middle—class children you can use more words and there isn't this problem of having to interpret for them because they pick up the words at home. I imagine they have trouble because their parents Speak in one—syllable words and they don't even make sentences. I can't get them to make a sentence. Even if I have them repeat after me exactly, they don't do it. They repeat in sentences they are familiar with. They're not really sentences but fragments of sentences that are familiar to them, and they understand them. They don't realize that they aren't making a complete thought. I think the children understand more than they speak. They learn things from TV. I don't think TV hurts them; they learn and learn but they don't use it. 79 Teacher Interview Vocabulary # 10 I think their vocabulary, to a great extent, is very limited when you compare it to a middle- class child. They don't come in contact as often with these words. Their parents seem to have a very limited vocabulary and this affects their vocabulary. They use terms of their own, such as, mash the light, for turning off the light switch. They don't mash the light switch, they mash the light. 11 The vocabulary is definitely limited; they speak in single words, simple words, not sentences. 12 They only use those words to which they had been accustomed to hearing. And, of course, we try to enlarge upon that in various ways. I used a large amount of materials. We would play telephoning and the child had to answer in complete sentences to try to get him to enlarge his vocabulary in various manners. 13 They use one—syllable words. They are like children who have no training at home. 14 Most of them had, on the average, an extensive vocabulary. One or two might have had a vocabulary of say a second or third grader. They got their vocabulary from older brothers and sisters but not from their parents; this is the difference. Television was a great influence for most of them. For example, most of them knew the TV commercials, and, as soon as they would see a particular type of cereal for breakfast they would say, "We saw this on TV." 15 The vocabulary of the total group is very, very limited——yes—-very, very, limited. This is why we must go into so many different explanations of a word. 16 If the vocabulary is something they have actually had experience with, then they know the word fairly well. If it's something they're not too familiar with, they will try and find a way of describing it, but they really won't come out with the exact word. Teacher Interview # 17 18 19 20 21 80 Vocabulary In the inner—city, the child's vocabulary is very limited. His experiences are very_limited. If you say, "Tell me what you see," the child begins with "It is something." He really has no name for it. If you have a lawn mower, he could say, "It's something you cut the grass with," or "It's a grass cutter." He will give you what the machine would do rather than its name. There's very little identification. I feel that they don't question for they have very little vocabulary with which to question. You see, if they have no words with which to ask a question, they can't question. He sees nothing for his experiences are so limited. He takes no vacation, has had no exposure to a real farm. Consequently, when we teach,every single experience we present has to be backed up with all kinds of pictures, all kinds of details so the child really has some kind of mental picture as to what we're talking about. It's hard to relate to this kind of child because he has nothing to base it on. We read about, for example, rabbits. Last week we had a real, live rabbit in the room and the child got a chance to feel it and to see the fur. The child learns in this way—-by seeing and doing. That which he sees and does--some of that he retains. I haven't noticed their vocabulary very much. I only noticed that it is very limited. I noticed that a couple of children wouldn't talk at all when they first came. After having something to talk about, if they were playing with a certain toy, they would find some— thing to say about it. Soon they would tell about things they were doing at home. They had a great deal of slang vocabulary. Other- wise they only used a few single words. They point at things instead of saying the word for it. We have to work with them to get them to talk and converse with us, other leaders and even the children. If you let them continue doing it, they would go on pointing out things rather than saying their names. Teacher Interview # 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 .30 81 Vocabulary I don't think they can communicate because they cant really let us know what theyire thinking and how they feel about things. You can hardly accomplish too much with a child unless you really know how they feel, how their mind works and how they're really thinking. Some of them have very short vocabularies whereas others who have had more experiences have more extensive vocabularies. In using descriptive words their vocabulary is very limited. They find it very difficult to describe things. What they do know they get from TV. It's the key to everything, especially words on "Batman" they know. These children know only very simple little words at first. They improve a great deal as our days together go on to the kindergarten. Few words are really known by the Head Start child, at least when they are speaking to the teacher or in school; they seem to get along all right when they are talking together. To be perfectly honest about it, I have a very hard time trying to communicate with these children. Forgetting the social differences, the children are simply language-starved. When the children are playing together they seem to understand each other, but the teacher is a different matter. It takes a long time of working together to be able to really talk with much sense. Most of their answers are one-word answers. These children are almost foreign to words used by middle—class children their same age. I think their vocabulary is quite limited because they have limited experiences in their environment —-their home environment. that is, until thev come to school. This is a new world for them. School words of teachers and books introduce completely new words to them-—in great contrast to Words they hear at home. 82 Conclusions of the Teachers' Concept of the Child's Vocabulary. --In brief review we find these statements often volunteered by teachers when they evaluate the child's vocabulary: limited—-very limited—-definitely one-syllable words meager vocabulary lack of vocabulary they point they gesture not exposed to many words understand more than they speak don't know the names for things don't talk at all short vocabularies they haven't heard the words. Strangely enough, however, a few teachers indicated tfliat they felt the children had rather extensive vocabularies, twat these teachers were fewer in number than those who felt the opposite. anJnmer Problems of Children: ngunents Most Often Used Teacher Interview # 1 Teachers' reaction to the question: What do you think are the major problems your children have with grammer . . . ? Grammar Their grammar is very, very different and it's limited and poor. These children have no idea what a sentence is. I don't think they have ever really realized that we put our words in sentences. They talk in short, little phrases. I try to get them to say their thought in whole sentences but they don't under— stand what you're saying. Like they say one word or they say "Yah." They say seen for saw, such as in "I seen him do this." Teacher Interview # 4 10 ll 83 Grammar I have given them exercises in grammar, like distinguishing which verb to use with singular nouns, which verb to use with plural nouns for I don't think their grammer is up to par. This is the reason that I stress this so much in my own classroom. I think the grammar has been very poor among these children, and there again, it's what they are used to hearing in the home. Their grammar problems are many because they use substitutions, this for that. They use a personal pronoun after a name and leave off the s, as in "he see." The biggest problem that I've had so far is "I'm gonna," "fishes" finr'TisNL "milks" for "milk” and foods" for ”food." The grammar is so poor that I use some of the few brighter children in the class to kind of trans- late for them because there is a real communication difficulty. I have to translate everything into their own language and they don't always under- stand me and I don't understand them at all times. Then I have to use an interpreter. They try to imitate the grammar--sometimes. They begin to try to help each other, too. I know one thing that "bugs" me. When I say "Where can I get a pencil?" they will answer, "Here it goes." It is hard for them to say "Here it is," but if I talk enough.about it, they may change. I would say their grammar is probably very poor. Where we would use a sentence to convey a thought, they are in the habit of maybe using a phrase or just a few words to try to convey the same thought which I would presume would affect their communication to a great extent. These children out words off: "could" would be "ould," such as in "Ould you like to do this?" Too, their "1's" were often missing. I really don't think it was because the child was unable to say the word; I would say it was just habit. Teacher Interview # 12 13 14 15 16 17 84 Grammar I would say here it's an environmental thing. They had used only those things which they had heard in their immediate environment. Some of these children had not even been out of their homes. They had not been downtown. They had not been any farther than their family circle. You could tell.right away if a child had been exposed to a wide vocabulary and good grammar. You could separate the children just by observation and listening. I've been working here for a long time so.I guess my ears are tuned to a great many of these forms of grammar--but I know they wouldn't be correct among friends of mine. The majority of the children don't speak in com— plete sentences. They might give you a staccato answer of one or two words. In phrases they 'might say something like, "That's he ball," instead of "That's his ball. To try to help their grammar I play games with the children. They have to supply the missing words or phrases, such as in, "Close the . . ." and they will say "door, " or "window." They are very alert when you make it fun for them. In grammar the children actually say one word instead of a complete sentence. We are working to have them say a complete sentence even though it's simple and short. Their grammar is terrible. "Her hit me," "tie me shoe" are examples of how the inner-city child speaks. This is because there is very little com- munication between the child and the parent. Now with the era of television, even though it plays an important part as far as learning is concerned, the child does not have to listen. Mickey Mouse is there. The motions take the part of the real listening. Because there is no real honest com- munication between parent and child, the child isn't taught to listen. He doesn't hear; he doesn't enunciate, you see. Teacher Interview # 18 19 2O 21 22 23 214 255 85 Grammar I find with my little ones at school that they always seem to put "me" instead of "If in the phrases they say. When they're speaking, instead of putting the other fellow first, they'll put themselves first. They'll say "me and my brother." I find this very frequently. Very few talk in sentences. Most of them are one-word comments, "yes" and "no" become "uh-huh" and "uh-uh." Some of them speak in full sentences, but they are only sentences of two or three words. I introduce the verb to children as an action word showing them what they're doing and the noun as the name of the person or place. That helps them write and speak in a complete sentence. They usually use single words or a phrase rather than a complete sentence and sometimes they would throw in a single word to answer the question. Disadvantaged children do not use complete sentences. Some of it, if I may say so and I'm reluctant to say so, does come from the home. If they don't hear a lot of good grammar they don't speak it--and they don't hear it at home. I find all children love to hear stories--especially these children who haven't heard many of them in their homes. I read to them a great deal and hope they listen to good grammar, which they are not capable of when they come to us. I often can't tell just how good their grammar is for they don't say much at first. I guess they are a little bit scared, I suppose. Their parents probably said, "When you get to school your teacher won't let-you do that." That is quite common, you know. I wouldn't mind if I heard pre—schoolers use the word "ain't" or misuse English for I just wish they would talk! Except for the very bright ones; I just wish they would express themselves in any way. Teacher Interview # 26 27 28 29 30 86 Grammar The greatest faults that any pre-schoolers have with grammer is the use of me incorrectly, see for sees or the use of sentence parts. Just get the children to talk, talk, talk, that's my big problem. I can teach them correct grammer if I can get them to feel at home in school. I'm just sure they are never spoken to in their homes to any great extent, that is why these children do not know correct grammer. You don't know what you haven't heard. The child's grammer is poor in terms of adult talk. It does get her to communicate with her little childhood friends, however. The children try to answer as quickly as they can so they use words instead of sentences. They're not concerned about grammar or maybe speaking in complete sentences or even thinking in terms of saying right or wrong——if they do know right from wrong. They're just trying to say, as_ quickly as possible, the answer they think you want and then they shut—up. Conclusions of the Teachers' Concept of the Child's Grtunmar.—-In brief review we find these statements often volninteered by teachers when they evaluate the child's gr ammar : poor grammar seen for saw words are spoken, not complete sentences he see for he sees child out off the ends of words her hit me or tie me shoe is a common example mg for I often get children to talk to teach_good grammar children's grammar is hard to determine for they don't speak much. 87 Pronunciation Problems of Children: Comments Most Often Used Teachers' reaction to the question: What do you think are the major problems your children have with pgonunciation . . . ? Teacher Interview Pronunciation # 1 Most of them don't talk at all when they first come to school. We talked to them; we tried to find out things they were interested in. We read stories. [No definite comments about.pronunciation.] 2 I would say on the whole their pronunciation of words was very poor. They leave off last sounds, leave off beginning sounds sometimes. But then I have that trouble now even with the older children. I keep saying to them to put in all the letters for that's why they're there. 3 Pronunciation is poor. Things like, "I wanna go," or "punkin" for "pumpkin and things like that. Their dialect is just hard to.understand for most teachers. We were born and raised in the Midwest, for the most part. 4 In reading, my children are not able to distin— guish beginning sounds very well and we stress this in first grade. This is what I stressed in Head-Starte-phonics is the answer. We do a lot of work in a play program with phonics daily. 5 Pronunciation is the main factor. The children seem to run the words together; they mumble and don't speak clearly. I had great difficulty under— standing some children who were in the program, not many, just a few. 6 They leave off the endings of words; instead of "going" it's "goin." Also the dfs and 3's give them trouble. Even at the beginning of words you often cannot hear the beginning letter. 7 The hardest sounds for the boys and girls are the g, s, gh_and.§t. I've been working on the phonics charts with them and then I send the word list home with them to practice. We listen for words on TV programs and these things have a lot of influence on them. Teacher Interview # 8 10 ll 12 il3 14 88 Pronunciation There is a definite problem of pronunciation and also the understanding of sounds, but the sounds are very difficult to pin-point. I know some of the other teachers have that same problem because I was called out of my classroom a few times to come and interpret for another teacher—-and to think we are all speaking English. I don't have any trouble making myself understood but some of the children withdraw when you can not make out what they are trying to say. Some of the words give them trouble, I'm sure, for they have never heard them spoken by many adults before they came to school. Looking back I don't think I had trouble talking to, being understood or understanding of the children I taught. They do have trouble with the pronunciation of word endings. I think that they're in the habit of not saying the things as clearly as we do and they say a word such as "looking" by leaving the g_off. They abbreviate to a certain extent. I wasn't familiar with the Southern background of most of these children and so I wasn't accustomed to their type of speech. They cut off their words. I would notice substitutions with regards to the initial consonants and at the ends of words. The dialect form, which I would gather they had from their parents and environment, was hard to under- stand, too. I think their pronunciation errors are errors that most children make. An example is the word git. I worked on rhyming words to get them to see that it wasn't gitf- or the word sing. They had a tendency to say sang. I think the high school girls were guilty of these errors, too. Together we made a few improvements in the speech of the Head Starters. We did lots of work with phonics. Some of the children had problems with their consonants, particularly at the ends of words. We had two or three problems of children who couldn't Speak at all. Teacher Interview # 15 16 17 18 19 2O 21 89 Pronunciation Let's see; they don't usually pronounce the last part of a word or letter. They slur over every- thing. They just pick it up at home. Many times they mispronounce because they do not know the sounds. They leave off the ending sounds and beginning sounds. The child hasn't really been taught to listen. Consequently he comes and gives us part of the word. There is no ending and very seldom a beginning. The word "get" becomes "git" and it's hard. You try to get the child to see that you can rhyme—-"let, met, set, and bet"--then when you get to the word "get" you still get "git." You see, it's a speech pattern that has been developed in the home, hearing the parents talk at home, brothers and sisters talk at home. They come with very little real listening. They hear no sounds at the beginning; they hear very few sounds at the end. I found this to be a problem. I found much dropping of endings. Then they sub- stitute a great deal. They say "slip" instead "Flip," a dog's name. This happens with the con- sonants a great deal of the time. They omit sounds at the beginning; they substitute sounds and particularly at the end of words they leave off the end sound. The "thfl sound seems to be a hard one for them. The "ing" sounds are usually left off, too. And also the"§" at the end of verbs gives them trouble. For "ask" they say "aks". They say "messing me" or "messing with me" which is poor grammar and slang and they don't even pronounce the "g" on H ing . I! They do have trouble with pronunciation for they fail to use their teeth and tongue and their lips. This is necessary for getting the correct sound. One boy's trouble was that he said "Gwadis" for "Gladys." Through showing him how to place his tongue he was able to say it. I used the mirror for him to see the placement of his tongue. Teacher Interview 55’ 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 9O Pronunciation Many of them are still talking baby—talk at that age. They communicate with movements, with their arms and gestures. They cut off the ends of words but I think a lot of adults are guilty as well as bigger children of this, too. Children seem to understand one another without the use of words. They have some problems with beginning consonant sounds and grammar which comes mainly from the home. These children have trouble with "gag, d_and t." We work with rhyming words and with beginning consonants and they just can't hear them. I keep repeating it and saying that a rhyming word is a word that sounds like the same word--sounds the same—-and they just can't hear it. I have one child who mispronounces almost every word, but they say he does not have a speech problem. All I have to say or do is to show the child how to pronounce a word and he'll say it right. You do have to deal with children differ- ently but they just want a little warmth and attention from adults which they just don't get at home. The children confuse the digraphs, for example: "th" and the "wh" too. They are not able to hear the sounds and other consonant blends. I just do a great deal of work with the children and phonics games, particularly rhyming. This way they gradually seem to hear the difference in the sounds. It gets very discouraging at times for I don't understand the children and I guess they don't understand me either. I work on phonics every day stressing the consonants, especially on the ends of words. I think these children know they don't speak so that teachers can understand them and so they really don't speak much at all. When they do they say things in a hurry——cut off all their words and last letters. 91 Teacher Interview Pronunciation # 30 I don't think the children are aware of the fact that they drop the endings of words. They talk quickly and slur off endings without even being aware that they're wrong. This is something that teachers really have to start working on. Many teachers use phonics constantly and they need to drill to try to help children enunciate the way they should. The different vowel sounds can be taught to children this way. Conclusions of the Teachers' Concept of the Child's Pronunciation.——In brief review we find these statements often volunteered by teachers when they evaluate the child's pron- Q unciation: ’ phonics is the answer--more and more phonics and more drill in phonics pronunciation is very poor they can't distinguish between Q's and 3's the ing at the end of words is cut to in hardeSt sounds are c, s, 22 and st can't understand the children and the children can't understand the teachers it is pronounced for at ‘ rhyming helps some children pronounce the letters they substitute letters trouble with beginning and ending consonants trouble with the many different ways of pro— nouncing the vowels they cut off words to get the talking over with they talk quickly and slur. Teachers' Responses to Question 2 At the same time that the teachers were asked to give tlieir opinions about child speech in terms of vocabulary, grmmnax. and pronunciation, a two—part second question was aSked~ These answers are recorded as follows: fi-fi a Wmfimw-w_ 3‘? - — - a 7 92 Reactions to Parents' Influence on ‘Child's Speech: Comments Most Often Used In what ways does the language of the parents influence children in your class? What prob— lems with vocabulary, grammar and pronunciation in the language of the parent are reflected in the problems of the child? Teacher Interview Reaction to parents' influence on child speech # 1 The parents' grammar and vocabulary is just as bad as the child's. Even the high school graduate mother who worked with me in.this program mispronounced words and her spelling wasn't good either. This mother said."feets,"."wa§" for "were" and "these".fora"those." Her child made the same mistakes. Most of my children have the same problems as their mothers. 2 One parent I know very well is going to college and she speaks well and so does her little boy. The other parents have many speech problems. I have found that if the child speaks well and has a large vocabulary, when the parent comes in, you can understand him right away. You can see the relationship between the child and.the parent. I have one boy who has a severe speech problem. He can barely be understood and when his mother came in I noticed the same speech problem.with her, too. I can hardly understand a word they say. I have to say to them, "Now slow down," and then it's a little better. They speak quickly and miSpronounce everything. It's discouraging. 3 I definitely found that the parents spoke much as the child did. If the parents spoke with a decided accent, then the child came to school with a similar sound. I noticed this lazy pronuncia— tion among parents as well as among the children. Their grammar and vocabulary was much.the same, although parents try to say little to teachers. They seem a little afraid of the school atmos- sphere. 4 The language of the parents would naturally greatly influence the children in our classes because, of course, the children have been with parents some four or five years before they come to us. This Teacher 93 Interview Reaction to parents' influence on child speech # is a difficulty that all teachers have in trying to improve the language of the children. This poor language of the parents is exemplified during the parent-teacher conferences. I feel sometimes that the parents are afraid to speak out. Because of this fear they don't speak as well as they would otherwise. This is possible. When children have limited vocabularies, the parents have limited ones. Even the "colorful" language of the home is sometimes spoken by the child-—either slang or profanity, I mean. Children are like their parents, very definitely, not only in their grammar, spelling and pro- nunciation but also in their every-day habits and attitudes toward the school and teachers. I think up to now parents have been afraid to come around to the school. With these new pro— grams the parents are coming to the school more often and they realize that they-have always wanted education for their children. They always felt that we were, many of us, a little stand- offish from the home of the child from the dis- advantaged area. Fathers very seldom come-~if they are in the home at all; it's always the mother. Now that we are establishing a closer feeling between the school and the poorer home we may be able to help the parent communicate better herself. In fact one mother asked me where she could take an English course and I was very encouraged for she desperately needs it. I feel that the home is one of the most important institutions in society and that actually the parents are using the same type of poor speech. So often we get the same pattern from the parent. I find that the parents' vocabulary is very limited and they are shy about talking to other adults for they don't know the words to use in expressing their thoughts—-just like their children. This language problem and the poverty in which so many of them live that they just cannot rise above it is the cause of the lack of stimulation in the child. Teacher 94 Interview “Reaction to parents' influence on child speech # 7 10 The parents I know are not too cooperative with the school. It seems they pick on the children. When-they speak to their children they curse whenever and wherever they feel like it. I had some who came to school and used words I wouldn't repeat. »My children.have many speech problems but if you were in their homes you wouldn't be surprised. The parents don't have many healthy experiences themselves and as the children aren't exposed to good.language they just don't use it. Speech classes are helping some of my class; now if we could only send the mothers to class, too. Parents have a great deal of influence on the speech of their children but so do their play— mates and the peOple in the neighborhood that they're talking with all the time. I don't know if it's possible to teach.the parents and the children. I don't like to call it standard or substandard for I think that they should learn one and it should be that used by the educated community in which they live. If that's standard, that's what both parents and children alike should learn. Parents have the great influence they do because the home is where the children learn in the first place, from the people they associate with. Maybe we should teach the children and then.they could influence the parents, but I don't think we can have as much influence on the parents as the children. To get ahead in the world the children will have to communicate on a higher level than their disadvantaged home. I think both the parents and the children have more influence on each other than they realize—— and the TV influences them both. If only the child would come to school with some knowledge of words or some interest in speaking-up, our job would be easier. When the parents mumble, the child mumbles. When the parents slur, the child does, too. ‘ I would say that parental influence is probably 99% of the problem. I think I can see why the children have problems because the parents have the same type of problems. Teacher 95 Interview Reaction to parents' influence on child.§peech # 11 12 1L3 I think I know what to expect when talking to a parent. I cut my own vocabulary down. I use only certain words and talk differently than I would to another teacher. During parent—teacher conferences I have to be aware of what word I am going to use here; you have to choose the right word in order for parents to understand you. The teacher has to put himself on the same Speech level as the parent and I've learned how to do that. My parents did a great deal of talking, but their Southern accents were hard to understand, just as it's hard to understand some of the children. - The speech of the parents and the children is i— so similiar that I feel it is rather difficult ' to change the child's pattern of speech in school. How can-you change a child's pattern when they're returning to a different kind of speech in their homes. But somewhere along the way we have to give that child the ability and skills to make that change. But when? In the limited vocabulary and bad grammar you can tell from the child just what type of educa- tion and what background the family has. The children who Spoke more frequently and in better phrases or even sentences were those who had parents with a greater vocabulary. Some of my parents had finished high school and could speak well but most of my parents didn't even finish grade school and you could tell the difference in their Speech. The parents had a very limited vocabulary and often substituted letters incorrectly in words as they Spoke to me. AS far as grammar is concerned, I found that it was about "even—steven." Some of them used very good grammar and some did not. There were words which they did not know which they would try to use—~just exactly as their children would do. I think the influence of parents on children is possibly more than we could even imagine—- especially the very young child. Most of the children Spoke exactly as their parents did. We couldn't get to the parents very well so we decided that we would try to help the children Teacher 96 Interview..Reaction to.parents' influence on.child.§peech # 14 15 16 I17 I with little games and say, "When you go home tonight, why don't you do this and do that." We were trying to get the parents interested and also to know what we were doing in school and yet not think we were "butting-in." The children in my class were more influenced by the speech.of their older brothers or sisters—— more than their parents for their parents were too busy either working or taking care of their household duties to really take time to talk to the children. Most of the parents are quite cautious about talking to us until they gain.confidence in us. Most of the speech problems of parents have to be dialect. Their grammar dealt mainly with singular and plural uses of verbs, such as: when to use "is" and "was" and these types of words." The children are in contact with parents more than teachers so the influence of the home is greater than that of the school, in my opinion.r Some of the children are deprived of talk with adults because there are so many children in the family. But then again, it's a group, too, in the family. What one says, the others follow. You know, it's a pattern that they follow and it's very hard to cope with. And it's very hard to change. I think that the language of the parents does influence the children because they pick up things that the parents use, expressions or ways of saying things. The children and the parents reflect each other, at least when the children are small. If a parent would mispronounce a word, the child tends to mispronounce the same word by copying the parent. That's natural. You can.tell which is the child's parent when they both say things like, "ggpfl for "then." It is true that a course in phonics could help both the parent and.the child. They eliminate the "pp" sound in most words, but in addition they mis- pronounce the.g's and the 3's. They say seen for paw, If both the parents and the children could learn to listen--to rhyme "pigngig, and gig with di ," they would be able to pronounce words more accurately. Teacher Interview 97 Reaction.to parents' influence on child speech 18 19 2O 21 22 23 Now this is what I meant by more or less their range in vocabulary. If the parent's vocabulary is limited, this will limit the child's. I have one little boy and his father who often comes to the school. This father speaks better English than most of the other parents and the boy who is his son speaks well, too. I think the parents most definitely will influence the children's pattern of speech. The child picks up what he's learned from home and carries this into the classroom. I've talked with several parents of the children in my class and I've noticed the same kinds of things the children say. I've heard them come from the parents when I've talked to them. I would say that there is a great similarity in the parent and child's speech. There is an obvious carry—over from the home to the school in almost everything, and this is particularly so in speech habits. The limited vocabulary of the parent was the reason why the child spoke so little in class or when he did his vocabulary was simple and his grammar was usually wrong. I think parents affect the child's speech by ig— noring it. I really believe this because whatever the child says, the parents accept it as correct. Maybe they know better, maybe they don't. I do think there is communication in the home. I think there is quite a bit of communication with the children because a lot of them come into school bubbling over to talk. When we have talking time, they don't want to stop. Although I do have some who aren't participating as they should; they aren't talkers——but neither are their parents. Mother's vocabulary, pronunciation——or lack of pronunciation, that is, dropping off endings, poor grammar, grammatical construction, are the same as the child's. They don't say "died" they both say "passed." I think children speak the way their parents do. And this, of course, is foreign to some teachers' ears. We try, you know, to correct it in school. But we can't Spend all of our time correcting it because then you never get the child to communicate. If only we could help the parents, but I guess that would be impossible. Teacher 98 Reaction to parents' influence on child Speech Interview # 24 25 26 27 The pronunciation of words on the part of the parent are reflected in the words of the child. Definitely, they pick up speech habits as they do all kinds of habits before they ever come to school. If they have very much communication at all, it‘s with their immediate family. So if it's evident in their parents, naturally the children will adopt it because, you know, they learn speech from their parents originally. The main communication the children have at home is not with their parents but with their television sets. When you ask them to relate experiences at home as in "show—and—tell" they only know what they saw on TV the night before. The influence parents have on children is terrific ——for good or bad. Both parents and the children make bad grammatical errors-—or slang expressions, I guess you would call them. They'll say, for instance, "He's messing around with me" or ”I'm a fixin to crack you." I heard curse words in this class, too, and it's not the kind of language we use in school. It might be picked up in the neighborhood or in most cases probably the home-- they repeat what they often hear. I make it a game to see if I can pick out the right parent for the right child and I'm not always right, but I often am. I just have to listen to their speech and if I know the children well, the association isn't hard to make. They cut off the same words and have the same dialect in their pronunciation of words. When I hear parents speak for the first time, I think to myself, "Like mother, like daughter," for they sound so much alike. I don't know whether this is an argument for heredity or environment; I guess it's both. When the home doesn't com- municate well in the sense that the parent can't talk comfortably with anyone outside their own home or circle of friends, this usually means he does not want to raise himself——or that he doesn't know how. We could just let children multiply this through the generations if we didn't try to raise the ambitions of the child. 99 Teacher Interview Reaction to parents' influence on child Speech 28 When children have trouble with the endings of words, so do their parents, it seems. Just being an observer of parents and their children, I would say their influence is great when the children are young, but as they grow older, their friends (other children) influence them more. When the influence of teachers occurs, I don't know. 29 The parents and children seem to be able to com- municate their ideas between themselves and probably in their home; it's just when they get into a different environment as the school that they have problems. Yes, I would say the speech problems of the child are directly inherited from the parent. 30 Oh, yes, I think the child Speaks very much like his parent does. And I think he speaks very much more so in this inner-city environment. I think the parents that have just come up from the South have a terrific southern accent which the children bring to class with them also. This is all they know so this is the way that they speak. Actually most parents just say "yes" or "no" and smile pretty——if they can possibly get away with it. Conclusions of the Teachers' Concepts of the Parents' Inflnience on Child Speech.—-In brief review we find these Staizennents often volunteered by teachers when they evaluate a parerit's influence on child speech: children speak the way their parents do pronunciation of parents' words are the same as those of their children Slang and curse words of parents are repeated by child cutting off letters from words are habits of both teachers and children communicate among their family members, but not as satisfactorily with teachers grammatical errors are the same for the child and his parent 100 the same errors are found in parent and child speech, such as, trouble with E's, Q's, ipg or word beginnings home influence on Speech is great .Reactions to Child and Parent Speech on Tape: Comments Most Often Used At this point in the interview the teachers were asked to listen to a tape of child and parent speech and ‘to react to both (see Appendix F for transcript of child 21nd parent speech to which the.teachers were asked to :react). Most of their reactions were facial expressions c>f the "I told you so" type or simply concentrated listen- .irig. The few representative remarks of some of the teeachers are recorded here in the exact language of the t eacher-informants: Teacher Language reactions of teachers who listened Interview to tape of child and pargit speech 1 (t<> children Poor enunciation, very definitely. They run 011 tape) the words all together. That's why we try to enunciate so well for these children. They have trouble with verbs; they have trouble with plurals. They don't understand. Even the parents don't understand the tense of verbs--the past tense or the present tense. They don't understand how to use the pronoun "I." They say "mg" and "pp“ when they ought to say "wg." The children don't know the names for things, even the names of common farm animals. Sometimes it takes even a long time to teach the colors to these children. I do think they have a knowledge of God. A little boy had disobeyed the school rules and a little girl said to him, "God won't love you." They have an idea that God loves and that's good. Teacher Interview # (to parents on tape) 2 (to children on tape) (tzo parents on tape) 3 (to children or1 tape) (to parents on tape) 101 Teachers' reactions to tapes You see, they don't enunciate either, and their Speech is limited; even the parent's speech is limited. They're hesitant. They're afraid to talk. They realize their Speech is limited; their vocabulary is limited and some— times it's hard to get the parents to talk to teachers at all. That child said, "My mama tooked her.” That is an example of their trouble with verbs and mixed up tenses. That's the past tense, but there is no such word as "tooked." They are so hard to understand; it's just like the time I have when I meet a new class of chil— dren. You just have to get used to the sounds of their words for they are so different from our own. You see that child is trying to tell the other child how to do something. They don't know how to tell them. They know how to do it but when it comes to telling people orally, they don't know how to get it across. They have a lack of vocabulary because of their little experience, probably. These parents talk just like the ones I know. Every other sentence they say "Cuz, ya know" or "yap." They keep doing this over and over. Did you hear that, "My daughter she" that's just the way the children Speak, too. The child says, ”my sister she went" or "my mother She." It was rather interesting but very wrong. The best thing about Head Start is that the children talk more and more as the time goes on. I noticed that about the parents who came to the school. Just like the parents on this tape. They seem to have a lot to say. That child said "I got me a bike.” I think that is a typical pattern; we heard this grammatical error many, many times. This sounds just like the same speech I mentioned before, words such as "visitin, projeck, hp gig" are ways of Shortening speech. They drOp the last letters on words, too. These mothers simply seem uninformed of the vocabulary for they seem to be searching for words that just don't come because they don't 102 Teacher Interview Teachers' reactions to tapgs # know them. It's strange though. They, the parents, that is, seem to get along very well with each other and have a lot to say. We're the outsiders when they get together for I surely can't understand them well enough to know what they're talking about. 4 This is so typical of what I experienced; you (to children know, I taught Head Start for two years. The on tape) type of answers the child gives and the teacher asking the question and not being able to really understand the answer is a perfect example of our class. The short, jagged words the child uses is a good example. They are not accustomed to listening to sounds, in fact, they are not accustomed to listening at all. This is the reason why I stress the beginning and ending sounds, and the middle of the word as well. (to parents That mother did not use the correct verb form. on tape) They don't seem to understand the use of verbs at all. This tape proved that the parents' speech is just as bad as the child's speech. We had a lot of searching for words in our (to children class just as these children are doing. They on tape) don't know the word for a particular article so they can't identify it. The only thing you hear a great deal about from these children, which surprised me, is God. They all go to Sunday School so they can tell you all about it on Monday. They must be allowed to talk there for they know the words and they really don't listen that well. They must discuss there. (to parents This sounds typical of many of our parents in on tape) the program, not being able to eXpress them— selves. I think they might have the idea; they just don't know how to put it into words. It is very frustrating for them——and for us. They have trouble pronouncing the words they do know, too. Listen to that mother; she puts S's on words when they don't belong there. T"This" becomes "dip." Teacher Interview # 6 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 7 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 8 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 9 (to children on tape) 103 Teachers' reactions to tapes That child's struggling effort to explain in how her mother makes a cake is just that--a real struggle. She has probably seen it many times but she doesn't know the simple words that could explain it. "That there thing" or her use of the word 'Stuff is very common among children in the inner—city area. I can say the same things for these parents as I did for their children. They don't have words enough to speak comfortably to the teacherS——but they do seem to be able to converse with each other, as they are doing, on this tape, that is. ”My mamma tooked her," that's a good example of the speech of the child in Head Start in my class. "I no gotta” is another typical saying. Rather than '3” they say "me" and mix other pronouns. These parents seem to get along better with each other than mine do. My parents seem shy with each other. These parents we're listening to have a hard time pronouncing words and even knowing enough words to say, but at least they are talking freely. This sounds more like my classroom than a tape of someone else's children. What a terrible time these children have searching for words in their poor, very limited vocabulary. Their pronunciation is poor; they all sound like they are from the South. These parents don't have words either, it sounds like. A lot of them don't have the experiences for one thing and then if they don't have the words, they can't really think about the experiences that they have had. They search for words but then I do, too, so that they can understand me. That child is Simply careless about what she says and how she says it. I think the parents just do not take enough time with their chil— dren to help them learn to speak well. This 104 Teacher Interview Teachers' reactions to tapes is just my theory. You have to encourage children to Speak by telling them stories or talking to them. That's why these poor children just don't know the words of the English language. (to parents If children ever imitate these parents they on tape) won't be much farther ahead than it they didn't hear words at all. The parents don't know vocabulary either. You can‘t communi— cate without words, can you? How do the ever get any directions over to their children? 10 They think they're really poking the words (to children out, it seems out of their mouths. That on tape) ". . . got me a bike" is a good example of their misuse of the pronoun. They're really slow; they're trying to search for words. They don't know exactly what word to use. (to parents They run their words together just like their on tape) children on the tape before them, don't they? There's another one, "mornin'; both the parents and the children alike cut off the ends of their words. 11 That "My mama tooked her”--isn't that typical (to children though? At least it's a good picture of how on tape the children in my class speak. And there is the word they use when they just don't know the correct word or name for a thing; they call it "stuff"; that seems to explain everything for them. (to parents That low-pitched voice of the mother is just on tape) like her child's that we just heard; you can't really understand either of them too well unless you make a Study of it. They chew up their words. 12 This is still an environment influence which (to children makes these children speak so poorly. From on tape) an uneducated parent you can't get children who know much. You see how they chop the words off? How the entire pronunciation of a word just isn't there? They continually substitute, just as they are doing here. The home has a terrific impact upon the child's ability to use grammar correctly, to speak distinctly and pronounce his words right. Teacher Interview # (to parents on tape) 13 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 14 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 15 (to children on tape) 105 Teachers' reactions to tapes Listen to his, "I like God." That was correct. I don't know whether this is due to environment or a physical thing, or a combination of both. I really don't think this is too bad for parents. They used the double subject, "my daughter she," but they all say that. This mother says "children," and most of the parents I know would say "chilen." A very common one is what this child just said, "My_mamma tooked her." And another one is "trainin wheels"; there is almost a com- plete omission of the g at the end of words. Their favorite word is "stuff" when they don't know the name of something. Do you think that tells us anything about vocabulary? I think there is a great deal of omission on the parent's part. They omit letters in words—— beginning, middle and end, just as they did on this tape. They can't use pronouns cor— rectly either. This child has lazy speech and has trouble with his beginning consonants. There is that word "stuff." I hear it from the children all day long for they just don't have enough words to use, I guess. Their dialect is so hard for me to understand. They say their letters in a completely differ— ent way-—besides mixing up the letters them- selves. Listen to that one mother; she says "£3" for the word "Egg." She would probably spell it right; She just says it differently than we do. The children have that southern "twang." You see, that is very prevalent around here, this same dialect. Sometimes it's very, very hard to understand them. They don't pronounce their pfs very much. They don't know words. They talk with their hands——and you can't tape that. They take for granted that their hands are going to tell you what they mean. Teacher Interview (to parents on tape) 16 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 17 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 18 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 106 Teachers' reactions to tapes This mother's voice is as low as most of the voices of my mothers. It's as though if they can't be heard, they can't be blamed for making mistakes. If only they had a little confidence we might be able to teach them something about how to help their children. They first have to help themselves. The children have the same problems all over this area of the inner—city. They use pro- nouns incorrectly, "me" for "I." They don't know verb tense and they shorten many of the words they say by dropping off a letter or two. That isn't saying anything about their poor pronunciation of words. The parents are just plain strange to the school environment and certainly to teachers. Their English is poor and they know it. I would be cautious, too, if I had such a limited vocabulary as these parents know they have. Not only is this poor, immature speech of children but this is completely disadvantaged speech. I mean that the children have never heard better language; no wonder they Speak the way they do. For example: "mamma tooked her," the word "stuff" which is supposed to stand for everything the child doesn't know by name. The mumble jumble of these parents is because they are afraid to speak up so that you would know they don't know good English. Even the words they do know are wrong. Just think of using, "she go," ”my daughter She"; those are just a few of the things they say incorrectly. These children speak so low and mumble so much that I sometimes think they are talking to themselves. Yet in this case they seem to ask little questions, don't they? They surely don't know verbs or pronouns. They just seem to be in a hurry to be over with it. They can't describe things at all. These parents mumble the very same way. If I had any doubt if they influence the speech of their children, this would really convince me for they speak exactly alike. Teacher Interview # 19 (to children on tapes) (to parents on tape) 20 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 21 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 107 Teachers' reactions to tepes The children in my class were just like these children. They talked very freely when they were talking about their toys, or their friends. The only trouble is they don't know enough words to really explain What they are thinking or feeling. The child here prob- ably Saw a cake or at least something being made at home, but She just couldn't talk about it in words. This tape must have been made of the parents sometime after Head Start began because they sound at home in the school—-even if their Speech isn't too good. At least they are talking; they didn't when they first came to school with their children. This is just the kind of speech I hear all day long here in my school. The children seem to want to talk but it's a hard task for some of them because they just don't know the words to say. "I'm gonna get me one" shows in just that one sentence that their pronunciation, verb and pronoun understanding is not right. You can see the carry—over here for the parent's English is as bad or Worse than her child's. The parents themselves are groping for words; you.can just hear them. There is a great similarity of parent and child speech on this tape. Yes, "My_mamma tooked her" is the ordinary way a child from the inner—city would talk. They don't know the difference between present or past tense-—or how to use the past tense. "Got me a bike" is the fast way to say some— thing. They don't know pronouns either. They either put in two subjects right next to each other or they skip them altogether. When I hear these parents talk, I'm more than ever convinced that the Head Start program is a worthwhile one. They certainly could not learn good language from parents such as these for their grammar is poor; you can't under- stand their pronunciation and their vocabulary is unbelievable. Teacher Interview # 22 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 23 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 24 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 25 (to children on tape) 108 Teachers reactions to tapes I think it's a good thing for children to get a socializing influence and this is what Head .Start has done for these little ones. You can just hear how they are talking to each other——even if the grammar is bad. The poor mothers——you can tell they haven't been to school too much or know how to speak our language correctly. Yet, they seem to be able to speak to each other well, don't they? This first child is an example of all the wrong things to say. For example, "gonna," "gonna get me," the wrong use of the verbs, tense and the pronouns. They cut off all the endings of their words, too, which is very hard on the person trying to make out what they're saying. I've taught in two different kinds of schools. One in which I taught upper-middle class children and this school of the inner—city in which I taught disadvantaged children. I know why the little inner—city children have such problems with English; their parents do not have good grammar, pronunciation or adequate vocabulary either. These children are talking freely enough but they just don't know the words and how to say them——either in sound or usage, I think. The best thing to do with children like these is to give them another or a better way of saying something rather than constantly correcting them. The parents speaking here are making the same Speech mistakes that my class of little children make all day long. They confuse verbs, pronouns and speak in a dialect I cannot understand. They do mispronounce an awful lot of words, don't they? I guess I forget sometimes for I've taught disadvantaged children so long that I've dropped to their level of Speech. I hope I don't make those errors, cutting off the ends of words, running words together and so on, but I may even do that at times. Teacher Interview (to parents on tape) 26 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 27 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 109 Teachers' reactions to tapes Parents make the same grammatical errors as their children. Now that I've listened to both tapes, I'm sure the children get their speech errors from hearing their parents Speak. I guess I haven't changed so much in my English or I wouldn't have noticed such glaring mistakes. My ears are just tuned to different dialects, I think. Oh, "My mamma tooked her" is a glaring mistake when we're listening to the tape, but actually this is not too unusual. I heard Similar remarks hundreds of times during the years of teaching disadvantaged children. They have the language of the streets and grammar is the least thing that disturbs them——young or old. Do you see what I mean now? The parents we've just listened to are very careless about their speech, too. When they want to make a point they use facial expressions, their arms, hands or any other part of their body which will Show pleasure or displeasure. The use of nouns and verbs is unimportant, and they seem to convey ideas to their own people. It is hard to understand these children on tape, but then it is just as bad in the class— room. They mumble their words so much and run on and on that it is almost impossible to understand what they say. I notice that they speak more clearly about their church on Sunday; they have little art lessons that they like, so maybe that's why they talk about it so much. Most of the parents of our children in this program are the same. They have good will but are almost illiterate, in my opinion. Only years of education and understanding will help these adults to help their children. Teacher Interview # 28 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 29 (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 30 . (to children on tape) (to parents on tape) 110 Teachers' reactions to tapes Yes, that sounds just like our little ones. '"Mamma she" and ”tooked her" are very typi— cal examples of the speech we listen to in this program and try to upgrade. I don't know how it's to be done, but an awareness of the situation should count for something. Most of these parents never finished elem— entary school, I'm sure, for they have come up to Detroit when they were just children themselves. They married young and had a flock of children and were too busy to worry about how they spoke or how they affected others. Verbs, pronouns, endings of words are just a few of their problems. These children speak so poorly because they have not had the chance to learn the correct use of their language. That first little girl sounds as though she is talking to her— self when she mumbling like that because she's probably doesn't have anyone at home that talks to her and draws her out. Maybe what I say about the children could be said about these parents of theirs, too. They seem almost afraid of the words they are saying. I think they really know they are not speaking like others, the teachers in the school, for example. I know these children are grammatically wrong in their speech but I am encouraged to hear them talking at all, at least so freely. These little children come to us with almost no experience with oral English and we have to start from scratch. I can teach them if only they respOnd. I don't think these parents are even aware of the fact that they are speaking so quickly and slurs slip in, too. The parents here are chopping off their words and so do their children. lll Conclusions of Teachers' Reactions to Tapes.——In brief review we find these statements often volunteered by teachers when they evaluate the taped speech of the child and his parent: speech is hard to understand children and parents sound just alike they both make the same grammatical errors they talk too quickly and mumble their words both the child and parent are afraid to speak in a new situation, especially in the school the speech problems of the child and the parents are foreign to the teacher who does not have these problems Teachers Characterize the Speech of the Disadvantaged Finally, the interviews were closed by the interviewer who asked each of the teachers if she or he could character- ize the speech of the disadvantaged. These were the simple replies they made: Teachers Comments 1. They don't talk much at all. 2. They slur words together. Their trouble is just Iggy speech. These people cannot distinguish between sounds. Pronunciation is the main speech problem they have. Their problems are communication and expression. Vocabulary is the biggest problem. The problem is just simple communication. \OGJVONUTJI-‘UU Just getting them to talk is the biggest challenge. 10. They mumble their words. Teacher 11. l2. 13. 111. 15. l6. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 2M. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 112 Comments Their background is the biggest trouble. The substitution of sounds is a concern. Dialect is the problem with these children. Just to get children to converse with each other is hard. They talk so fast they stutter. The major speech problem is pronouncing words. The biggest problem is that the child learned to talk from hearing his parents talk. The major problem besides their limited vocabulary is the dropping of endings. They omit sounds at the beginning; they substitute sounds, andleave off sound endings. Their pronunciation is bad. They point at things rather than say words. Their main ways of communicating are by movements of the arms and other gestures. The biggest problem is the grammar of the language. The misuse of the pronouns is the most obvious trouble. The hardest thing is to express their thoughts in complete sentences. A limited vocabulary is the biggest problem. They need to know more and more words. They speak so quickly they often stutter. They mumble their words. Their speech is just very careless and lazy. 113 Summary Individually taped teacher—interviews of the teachers' concept of the problem is noted in the reactions to the questions proposed in this chapter. In summary, the teachers' answers are tabulated to the following questions: 1. Is it true that there is a difference between the language of the children and the teachers? YES: 30 NO: 0 2. Is it true that there is a difference between the language of the parents and the teachers? YES: 30 NO: 0 It was mentioned by two of the teachers that they noticed that a few of their parents were high school graduates and some attended college and that the language of these parents was of superior quality to other parents in the program. The implication here is that the teacher felt that these parents could communicate with the teachers on a higher level of speech due to the more formal education enjoyed by these parents. 3. Is it true that there is a difference between the language of the parents and their children? YES: 0 NO: 30 The teachers also mentioned the influence of television, the neighborhood, their peers, the church and older brothers 11” and sisters on the speech of the pre-schoolers, but every teacher said she thought parents were a great influence on the child's language. N. Is it true that the teachers feel that the language problems of children are due to their disadvantaged social status? YES: 30 NO: 0 Every teacher mentioned that she felt that the disadvantaged social status of the children, and the parents as well, was the main cause for their langauge problems. A listening to a tape of child and parent language reinforced this belief and their estimate of the influence of parents on child speech. 5. Is it true that the teachers acknowledged that disadvantaged children do have a language system within their sub group language? YES: 0 NO: 27 PERHAPS: 3 Three of the teachers (#ll, 15 and 21) implied that there was some type of communication system the disadvantaged children use whereby they communicate with their families, friends, and neighborhood. This was not called a language system as such by any one of the teachers. The teachers seemed to feel that the majority of the pre-schoolers have definite speech problems in the areas of 115 pronunciation, grammar and vocabulary. In particular, pronunciation gave them the most-concern. The teachers referred to their repeated use of phOnic drills and word games in an effort to up—grade the child's pronunciation skills. This also helped the limited vocabulary that was considered a serious problem as well. The teachers used the phonic practice within the context of words, phrases and short sentences to insure language understanding and in an effort to help children identify sound differences. 6. Is there a way that teachers can instruct children in the acquisition of standard English skills? This question was not asked directly of the thirty teachers interviewed, but all of them indicated that they felt that the Head Start program was a worthwhile experience for disadvantaged children who have been deprived of certain basic language experiences that the middle-class child normally enjoys. Some of the teachers said that they used word games, phonic drills and rhyming stories with the children to perfect their listening skills and to encourage oral discussion. An identification and description of the language problems and a proposed solution to the situation were sought by the teachers involved. Chapter V presents an analysis of the Language Data and the Investigation in terms of the phonology, grammar and vocabulary of the pre—schooler, his parent and his teacher. CHAPTER V SOME INDICES OF SOCIAL STRATIFICATION IN DETROIT The speech of the informants of this investigation was tape recorded so that the language behavior of the pre—school child, his parent and his teacher could be described. The phonology, grammar and vocabulary of the participants was tabulated for individual and group study. In particular, the child informant was of first importance because of his status in terms of educational implication. The Child's Language Behavior In the search of the data and in line with the con— cern voiced by the teachers, as noted in Chapter IV, certain phonological indices were suggested. The tabulation of this data produced a social stratification1 of language performance in the various areas of speech study. Some Aspects of the Child's Phonological System To study the speech behavior of the 156 children informants in the investigation each child was given a lStratification means the separation of sets of characteristics into distinct levels; a second-order struc— ture. See Labov, The Social Stratification of English in New York City (Washington, D. C.: Center for Applied Linguistics, 1966), p. 581. 116 117 number 1 to 156; beginning with l, every third child was chosen for the sample up to 50 children, all of whom were included in this sample. The following phonological occur- rences were noted: The Deletion of the Stop Consonant /t/.——Of the 93 potential occurrences of final /t/ in the speech of Eggd S3133 children, there were 7“ deletions and 19 realized occurrences. This is a 79% deletion of /t/ and 21% realized occurrences. The following are examples of these deletions: don't 9 occurrences of the delection; l per 7 informants, 2 per 2 informants. faint l occurrence thought 1 occurrence coat 2 occurrences l per informant carrot 5 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant eight 8 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant oat l occurrence hurt I occurrence hot 9 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant right 1 occurrence toothpaspe l occurrence toast 1 occurrence basket 1 occurrence 118 dessert 1 occurrence can't I occurrence twist l occurrence west 1 occurrence light 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant cat 3 occurrences of the deletion; ‘ l per informant fort l occurrence peanut l occurrence point 1 occurrence out 1 occurrence paint l occurrence age 1 occurrence elephanp 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant chocolape l occurrence whipe 7 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant hop (dog) 1 occurrence heart 1 occurrence breakfasp l occurrence The Head Start children in this investigation were Dre—kindergarten age, approximately 5 years of age. The children in the speech sample of the Detroit Dialect Study were ages 9 to 12. The speech data of the language of older children in this same geographical and socio—economic group are the following: children from 9 to 12 years of age, 119 in the language research of the Detroit Dialect Study, delete the stop consonant /t/ in the final position U6% of the time, modify the final /t/ to a glottal2 stop 28% of the time, and realized the final /t/ stop 26% of the time.3 It must be said here that the speech sample of the Head Start children counted potential deletions of the final stop consonant /t/ and the realized occurrences of the deletions; the Detroit Dialect Study considered the glottal stops in addition.“ In addition, in the larger investiga- tion of the speech of Detroit, the Detroit Dialect Study discovered that all deletions of the stop consonant /t/ in the final position occurred before fronted vowels, such as in this example of lenis articulation: There is a mosquito; hit it.5 Though there is little research published in the area of the stop consonant /t/ in the medial position, this study attempted to note examples of this in the speech of Head Start children. Of the l7l potential occurrences of 2A glottal stop is produced when the vocal chords or folds are brought together sufficiently to obstruct the passage of air, but not to produce voice. See H. A. Gleason, Introduction to Descriptive Linguistics (New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston Company, 1955), p. 21. 3David Rickard (staff member of the Detroit Dialect Study), "A Progress Report of the Detroit Dialect Study" (an unpublished master's thesis, "Sociological Correlation of the Allophonic Variations of Alveolar Stops in the Speech of6Highland Park School Children," Michigan State University, 19 7). “Ibid. 5Ibid. 120 medial /t/ in the speech of these children, there were 35 deletions and 136 realized occurrences. This is a 20% deletion of medial /t/ and 80% realized occurrences. The following are examples of these deletions: 1. Simple /t/ deletion: a. /t/ +Q before vowel, as in potappes 2 occurrences of the deletion l per informant Sanpa l occurrence water 5 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant bupper 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant bupterfly 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant cppton l occurrence skapes l occurrence b. /t/ + C before b: foopball 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant c. /t/ + C before 1: bapple l occurrence 2. /t/ deletion in consonant clusters: a. /nt/ + /n/, as in: painping 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant Flinpstones-TV l occurrence pangs 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 121 b. /rt/ + /r/, as in: turple l occurrence shorts 1 occurrence dirpying l occurrence 0. /st/ + /s/, as in: sisper l occurrence muspard H occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant sppre l occurrence d. /kt/ + /k/, sd in: docppr l occurrence On the basis of final stop consonant /t/ evidence, it can be predicted that a similar phonemic deletion might occur in the instance of the medial position /t/, but the task is a difficult one to prove conclusively because of the slurred situation in the speech. The Deletion of the Stop Consonant /d/.--0f the 102 potential occurrences of final /d/ in the speech of Hng §Eg£t children, there were 52 deletions and 50 realized occurrences. This is a 50.8% deletion of /d/ and ”9.2% realized occurrences. The following are examples of these deletions: bird 6 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant red 15 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant playground 2 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant 122 H found occurrence Donalg (Duck) 1 occurrence bed 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant cold 5 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant old 3 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant head 1 occurrence food A occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant salad 1 occurrence mustard 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant friend 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant wood 1 occurrence dad l occurrence bad 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant good 1 occurrence record 1 occurrence According to the data of the Detroit Dialect Study, children ages 9 to 12 in the lower socio—economic class delete the stop consonant simple /d/ in the final position if the next word has a consonant in the initial position. Twenty-four per cent of all final /d/'s are substituted With /t/, in the speech of the lower socio—economic class and only in 5% of all final /d/'s in the speech of the upper socio-economic group. The past tense [D1] whether 123 it is realized as /t/ or /d/ phonemically as in walked, is deleted by the disadvantaged 30% before vowels, 36% before consonants, and substituted for 3% before vowels and 9% before consonants.6 Though there is little research published in the area of the stop consonant /d/ in the medial position, this study attempted to note examples of this in the speech of Head Start children. 0f the 58 potential occurrences of medial /d/ in the speech of these children, there were 19 deletions and 39 realized occurrences. This is a 33% dele- tion of medial /d/ and 67% realized occurrences. The following are examples of these deletions: 1. Simple /d/ deletion: /d/ + C before vowel, as in: mogel l occurrence sigewalk 1 occurrence nobody 1 occurrence cangy 2 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant Cagillac l occurrence 2. /d/ deletion in two consonant clusters: a. /nd/ + /d/, as in: friengs 1 occurrence sangwich 7 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant Mongay l occurrence 6Ibid. 124 b. /db/ + /b/, as in: goog—bye l occurrence 0. /dm/ + /m/, as in: Gogmother l occurrence 3. /d/ deletion in three consonant clusters: a. /ldr/ + /l/, as in: chilgren 1 occurrence b. /ndm/ + /1/, as in: grangma 1 occurrence According to the findings of the Detroit Dialect Study, /d/ cluster is deleted in the consonant cluster in the speech of the disadvantaged in 3H% of the potential occur— rences, and is glottalized or the /t/ consonant is used in substitution 4% of the potential occurrences in the speech of the lower socio—economic class.7 As in the studies of Labov in his work with the dis- advantaged of New York City, the frequent occurrences of v/t/ and /d/ consonant stop deletion might be expected. The same consonant stops were identified in the Detroit Dialect Study as well: It is a general characteristic of the lower socio-- economic class that there is at least lenis (lightly articulated) pronunciation for most consonants of all kinds so that you get complete deletion of 7Ibid. 125 8 stops, nasal consonants and fricatives. In many cases these are grammatical markers such as the —t, -d, and ng, which are phonologically conditioned not grammatically. Lenis pronunciation has worked its way down to deletion. ’ The Substitution of /n/ for /n/.--0f the 81 potential occurrences of /g/ in the speech of Head Start children, there was 66 substitutions and 15 realized occurrences. This is an 83% substitution and 17% occurrences in which /9/ was realized. The following are examples of these deletions: 1. -ing morpheme: running 2 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant skating 6 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant batting 2 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant cooking 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant riding 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant climbing 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant shopping 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant falling l occurrence looking 1 occurrence 8A fricative is a narrowly constructed consonant charac- terized by a continuous hissing or scraping noise, produced by turbulent motion induced in the air stream; the initial consonants of fin, vim, etc., and medial consonant of plea— sure. See Labov, The Social Stratification of English in New York Cit (Washington, D.C.: Center for Applied Linguistics, 19665, p. 579. 9William Riley (staff member of the Detroit Dialect Study), "Phonological Indices of Social Stratification" (unpublished master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1967). going talking playing doing sitting buying waving singing happening juggling reading painting making having fishing catching swimming bathing flying standing taking laying placing stopping ...J H ._l N H H H H ...—J '._l '—l }_l H H H l-‘ H H 126 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant occurrences of the deletion l per informant occurrences of the deletion; l per informant Occurrence Occurrence Occurrence occurrence occurrences of the deletion; l per informant Occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrences of the deletion; l per informant occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence 127 doing 1 occurrence getting 1 occurrence barbecuing 6 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 2. monomorphemic —ing: nothing 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant something 1 occurrence In most speech, nasal consonants are deleted at times; this is not necessarily a social marker, according to the findings of the Detroit Dialect Study. The deletion of the /m/ occurs far less frequently; only lower class socio- economic groups have this nasal consonant as a speech marker, as in them or home. Some Aspects of the Child's Grammar System Specific items of grammatical usage occurred fre— quently to thus form an obvious speech pattern among the pre—school informants. The "s" inflection.—-Three kinds of data are noted here: 1. Noun plurals: he didn't have no foods 1 occurrence he done broke his ankleg (ankles—meaning one) 1 occurrence 2. Verb 3rd singular: my mamma gotg l occurrence she say_ it 1 occurrence 128 I's scared l occurrence my sister got runs over 1 occurrence you know whats this 1 occurrence anything woulda's happened to me 1 occurrence my army mens gots stolen 1 occurrence that_ what my fathor do's 1 occurrence he see_ it 1 occurrence 3. Pronominal forms: playing by his self 1 occurrence my's cousins l occurrence Pronoun Redundancy Following the Subject of the Clauser—In a count of the instances of pronoun redundancy, it was noted that all six examples of this aspect of the child's grammar system included all the potential occur— rences; the child only used this form when referring to his family and when the personal possessive adjective was used before the subject. Examples of this aspect of the child's grammar system are as follows: 1. my cousin sps came 1 occurrence 2. my sister sps wants 1 occurrence 3. my mother sps goes 1 occurrence A. our baby gs cries 1 occurrence 5. my brother ps_tries l occurrence 6 my mamma sps_buys 1 occurrence 129 The often repeated use of the pronoun after the familiar name seemed to give the speaker the assurance that in this way the listener would be sure to understand the communication; the reinforcement was an attempt to assure this happening, in the opinion of the investigator. The Use of the Double Negative.——Of the 39 potential occurrences of double negative in the speech of Head Start children, there were 23 examples of its use and 16 realized occurrences. This is an example of use of the double ziegative among 58% of the informants in contrast to 92% of tlie informants who did not use this form. Examples of the use of the double negative in the sgbeech of the children are as follows (1 occurrence per 1 ixiformant is the frequency count): 1. he doesn't have gs pockets 2. we @2212 have pp more 3. that ain't no puppy A. that ain't no dog 5. he didn't say nothing 6. we gppr have pp grits 7. I @2212 got me ppps 8. there ain't no tractors 9. they didn't bake p9 cake 10. I gppr have pp skates ll. Mamma gppip buy me ppps 12. We dpp;t_have pp_corn flakes 13. I don't need pg help 130 14. I didn't have pp_birthday. l5. gpplp run pp more 16. he siplt_gonna do pp work 17. he didn't have pp foods 18. I siplp goin' to do nothing 19. he ain't no good 20. I slplp got pp shoes on 21. I sgplp goin' put his shoes pp where 22. Mamma sgpip_goin' buy me pp skates 23. they gppip got pp_more money The Detroit Dialect Study investigated this aspect of the child's grammar in the speech of disadvantaged children 10 to 12 years of age. The totals of actual double negation in relation to potential occurrences were given for each informant, arranged in the order of descending social rank. The following percentages were given, beginning with the lower socio-economic (or disadvantaged) children to the highest socio-economic group of children in the sample: Actual occurrences: 55.0% Lower socio—economic group 43.5% 6.9% 0 % Highest socio—economic group10 10Walter A. Wolfram (staff member of the Detroit Dialect Study), 'Tuutiple Negation and Social Stratification" (unpub- 192$?d progress report of the Detroit Dialect Study, July, 131 The frequent use of the double negative is a mark of the less academically educated member of the society, a member of the lower socio-economic class, whose lack of academic experience allows him the freedom of-grammatical usage, unhampered by rules of the language. It is not. surprising, then, how this less language-restricted social class uses the double negative freely in oral communication. The "done" Perfective Auxiliary —-The following uses of done were noted in the speech of the Head Start children: 1. Deletion of 3rd singular "s" inflection The following uses of done were noted in the speech of the Head Start children (1 occurrence per 1 informant): that's what my father gp_ my mamma gp_that 2. Auxiliary my brother gpps_gone he dppp_got a Cadillac I gpps go to the other yard he gpps_take his shoes off Mamma gpps_gone for a barbecue I dpps_had corn flakes I gpps_seen a cow I dpps seen a horse she gpps seen a pig he done sat down 132 Vocabulary of the Child As can be noted in the teachers' concept of.the lan- guage problems of the disadvantaged pre—schooler (see Chapter IV), the vocabulary of this child is extremely limited, even in comparison with his peer group, at the middle-class social level. A Although admittedly a representative study of the language problems of the informants rather than an exhaustive one, it is obvious to the investigator that the vocabulary of the participants in the study is different. The difference lies in lexical items just as is any impor— tation from any other part of the country, and many of the informants are natives of the Southeastern region of the United States (see Chapter III for biographical data of the informants). There are no immediately striking differences among the people in the sample. All persons can describe and narrate that which needs description and narration, in terms of their age group and academic experience. It has been said that some of the best language innovators are found in the lower socio-economic level of the population for they are not literarily restricted. Language Data of the Parents One of the concerns of this study was the language of the pre—schooler's parent. The mother of the child in the Head Start project was of prime interest because of the usual closeness of the mother and a child of pre-kindergarten 133 age. Because of this, the mother served as a language informant and her speech was recorded for descriptive study of her phonology, grammar and vocabulary. In a very few cases the father of the family served as the parent-informant, but these were rare. For the manner of parent-selection, see page 116 of this chapter for parents were chosen for the sample just as their children were. Some Aspects of the Parents' Phonological System The Deletion of the Stop_Consonant /t/.—-Of the 1A1 potential occurrences of final /t/ in the speech of the Head Start parents, there were 113 deletions and 28 realized occurrences. This is an 80% deletion of /t/ and 20% realized occurrences. .The following are examples of. O the parents' speech: couldn't 8 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant start 3 occurrences of the deletion; .' l per informant important A occurrences of the deletion; l per informant dentist 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant just ‘ 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant won't l occurrence quipe l occurrence don't 7 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant met 1 occurrence different A occurrence of the deletion; l per informant cutest can't breakfast paint most eight benefit hot right toast set taspe parent select carrot cat saint elephant west 13“ 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 6 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant A occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 3 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant 5 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant l occurrence- 5 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 3 occurrences of-the deletion; l per informant 2 occurrences of the deletion; the same informant 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per-informant 2 occurrences of the deletion; the same informant 8 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 2 occurrences of the deletion; the same informant 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 2 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 3 occurrences of the delection; the same informant A occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 135 east 3 occurrences of the l per informant whips 2 occurrences of the l per informant bipe l occurrence tint 3 occurrences of the the same informant must A occurrences of the l per informant let 2 occurrences of the l per informant. deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; The Detroit Dialect Study found that the adults from the lower socio-economic level deleted final /t/ 62% of the time when followed bv a consonant; they glottalized the final /t/ 31% of the time, and in only 9% of the cases are the final /t/'s realized occurrences.ll Of the 137 potential occurrences of msdial /t/ in the speech of the Head Stars parents, there were A2'dele- tions and 95 realized occurrences. This is 31% deletions of the medial /t/ and 69% realized occurrences. 1. Simple /t/ deletion: a. /t/ + E before vowel, as in: yesperday 3 occurrences of the l per informant lesser 4 occurrences of the 2 per 2 informants masperpiece l occurrence interested 5 occurrences of the l per informant lORickard, op. cit. deletion; deletion; deletion; 136 spars 2 occurrences of the deletion; the same informant poverpy 3 occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant /t/ + D before m, as in: resenpment l occurrence /t/ + C before 1, as in: lipple u occurrences of the deletion; l per informant ' " 2. /t/ deletion in consonant clusters: /nt/ + /n/, as in: fainping 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant painping 2 occurrences of the deletion; " l per informant /st/ r /s/, as in: misper A occurrences of the deletion; 1 per informant muspard 7 occurrences of the deletion; ‘ ' l per informant ' sisper 3 occurrences of the deletion; ' ' l per informant ‘ The Deletion of the Stop Consonant /d/.-—0f the 138 potential occurrences of final /d/ stop consonant in the speech of the Head Start parents, there were 8“ deletions and 9M realized occurrences. This is a 61% deletion of /d/ and 39% realized occurrences. The following examples are from the parents' speech: crippled blind find cried enjoyed scared helped pretend did attend remind forward robbed bed tired child dread kind friend ride asked 2 H H 11 H U.) .2 H H H l...) 10 137 occurrence occurrence occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the 1 per,informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the 1 per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrences of the l per informant' occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; occurrences of the deletion; l per informant 138 respond 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant changed 7 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant As has been said, the Detroit Dialect Study found that with both child and adult speech of the disadvantaged lower socio-economic group the final stop consonant /d/ is often substituted with the stop consonant /t/. Of the potential occurrences of medial position /d/, there were found to be 66 in this particular speech of the adult. There were 26 deletions and NO realized occurrences. This is 39% and 61% realized occurrences. 1. Simple /d/ deletion: a. /d/ + O before vowel, as in: bolger H occurrence kingergarten 8 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant everybogy l occurrence reaging 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant rides l occurrence megical l occurrence reagy l occurrence olger 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant b. /d/ + 0 before 1, as in: bungle l occurrence 139 2. /d/ in consonant clusters; a. /nd + /n/, as in: friends wonderful 1 l b. /dr/ + /r/, as in: bedrooms 1 c. /ds/ + /s/, as in: Edsel d. /ldr/ + /l/, as in: children The Substitution of /n/ for [D/,-—Of the 61 potential occurrences of /g/ in —ing morpheme of the speech of Head Start children, there were 48 substitutions of the /n/ for l 2 occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrence occurrences of the l per informant /g/ and 13 realized occurrences. This is 79% of /n/ for /g/ and 21% occurrences of realized /9/. deletion; substitution The following are examples found in the parents' speech: painting singing counting drawing complaining babying telling 2 |._J H occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrence occurrences of the l per informant deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; picking talking showing morning fighting skating going ending coloring eating coming being making wearing asking playing As with the that the deletion and occurs in the N F H C\ '—l H 11:0 occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrence occurrences of the l per informant occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrence occurrences of the l per informant occurrence occurrence deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; deletion; children, the Detroit Dialect Study found of /g/ is not necessarily a social marker speech of all groups at times. 141 Some Aspects of the Parents' Grammatical System Specific items of grammatical usage occurred fre— quently to thus form an obvious speech pattern among the parents of pre—school informants. The "s" Inflection.—— l. Noun plurals: I go everydayd l occurrence 2. Verb 3rd singular: my child enjoy_ it 1 occurrence I liked books 1 occurrence I telld him 1 occurrence he say_ 1 occurrence they gotd the same teacher 1 occurrence makeg sure that I reads it 1 occurrence I sayd l occurrence he say_ no 1 occurrence he tell_ me 1 occurrence teacher get_ mad 1 occurrence she see_ them 1 occurrence she do_ it I occurrence what teacher say_ 1 occurrence people talkd l occurrence I can workd l occurrence I startd l occurrence she like_ it 1 occurrence 142 she say_ 4 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant he say_ 7 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant Pronoun Redundancy Following the Subject of the Clause.——In a count of the instances of pronoun redundancy, it was noted that all seven examples of this aspect of the parent's speech included all the potential occurrences; the parent used this form when referring to his family and when the personal possessive adjective or proper noun was used before the redundancy. Examples of this aspect of grammar are as follows: the teacher she says my kids they go Carolyn she is five my daughter she cried my mother she says my little girl she my son he is happy The Use of the Double Negative.-—According to the Detroit Dialect Study, the double negative is a distinctively social marker in grammar: In the first place, there is one group of speakers for whom the "multiple negative realizations" are always absent. The chart indicates that these speakers.consistently evidence higher social rankings (i.e., lower social index scores) than those speakers who use the multiple negative.12 l2Wolfram, op. cit. 1&3 The Detroit Dialect Study found that the percentage of double negatives were found in these percentages among their adult informants: 7u.6% Lowest socio-economic group of adults (of all potential occurrences) 35.2% ‘ 19 01% O % Highest socio-economic group of adults This was found to be true, too, in the class of this study of adults, parents of the disadvantaged pre-schoolers: 78% of the adults in the sample used the double negative at least once as in contrast to 22% who did not. The ""done" Perfective Auxiliary.-—The following uses of done were noted in the speech of the Head Start children: he done it 3 occurrences of the deletion; l per informant I done it before 1 occurrence she do it 1 occurrence Carolyn done it 1 occurrence Vocabulary of the Parent It must be said of the parent as it was said of the child; many of the parents with admittedly very little academic experience or travel experience throughout their lives have extremely limited vocabularies. Many of the Questions proposed by the teacher or Interviewer were 14M answered by one word answers and many of these answers were 'either "yes" or "no," or words of a simple nature. Langdage of the Teacher The language of the teacher was tape recorded as was that of the other two informants, however, the results were not as fruitful. Whereas the pre—schooler and his parent were not as standard in the use of language as was the teacher, the pre—schooler and parent speech was of a casual nature. The teacher was much more on guard with the use of her language. It was noted that the teachers' speech, on the whole, was a careful and guarded language. The vocabulary was of an extended type, as one might expect. The grammar was traditionally correct, as a rule. The phonology was something else, however. As can be noted, a number of the teachers were not native Detroiters and had the regional phonology of their birthplaces (see Chapter III for the biographical data of the teacher— informants). Summary 1. The teachers' concepts of the language problems Of the children were correct in some instances and incorrect in others. They identified the deletion of the flag; COnsonant stops /d/ and /t/ correctly. 2. The deletion of the nasal consonants /ng/ Was also identified as part of the phonology of the dis— advantaged child in Head Start. This is a true identification, 145 however, it was not understood that in most speech nasal consonants are deleted at times and that this deletion is not considered a social class marker. 6 3. The grammatical identification of the use of the past participle "E292? for the simple present tense form of "sggj"or the use of the participle without the auxi— liary, was made by many teachers. This may be a phonologi- cal problem rather than a grammatical one. The only positive way one could be sure would be if the auxiliary were used in its complete rather than contracted form, i.e., "I have seen him." . H. Vocabulary was of major concern to the teacher yet no mention was made that this is an importation from another region of the country and is not in and of itself good or bad for that reason. Vocabulary differences are in degree. For example: the word "EElEE" is used by persons of various races and ethnic backgrounds; it is a word imported from the South of—the United States. 5. The words of the language are the items that seem to bother teachers most, but there are other aspects of language as important or more important for educational purposes. Structural things are the really crucial problems of language, not the most obvious problems which are the words themselves. There is a folksy feeling that lexicon is language, but that is Just one facet of the more complex study. l3Riley, op. cit. 146 6. It is important to know that social classes determine language. Speech is determined by the social community of which the individual is a member, not the race or ethnic group in which he is born, as proven in the studies of Labov.lu Chapter VI discusses the proposal that a standard English be taught to the child, to accompany the dialect that he uses easily with his peers. Biloquialism, it is suggested, will permit the child to retain his natural position in the family and peer group which speaks the sub-group language, while equipping him to meet the demands of a society which accepts standard speech as that which the educated man speaks with ease and confidence. This chapter considers the evidence of language behavior in terms of the actual data of the investigation and the teachers' concepts of the language problem. Recommenda- tions include suggestions to the teacher who would be expected to remedy language differences. These include proposals for pre-service and in—service training of teachers and curriculum revision in terms of the needs of children. luLabov, op. cit. CHAPTER VI CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS This investigation studies the question of the language differences which exist in the speech of the pre— school child in Head Start, his parent and his teacher. The project involved the taping of the speech of these three types of informants in a first inverview in the school. Some aspects of the informants' language behavior were analyzed and described in this manner. The descrip- tion of this speech is discussed in detail in Chapter V. A second interview was conducted with a sample of the teachers who served as informants in the first inter- view as well. The purpose of the second interview was to determine the teachers' concepts of the language problems of the pre—schoolers and their parents. Their reactions to the speech of the children and their parents, both from their personal classroom experiences and from listening to a tape of the children and their parents, have been described in Chapter IV. 147 1118 How Pre—School Children in Head Start and Their Parents Differ in Some Aspects of Language Behavior and the Teachers' Concepts of the Problem The language data of this investigation suggest that there are differences in the language behavior of the in- formants. Some of these differences and the teachers' con— cepts of the problem, both before and after she had listened to the taped speech,are shown on the following page. It must be said that the teachers were not specific about their comments concerning the pronunciation of the children and their parents, but all of the teachers acknowl— edged in some way that there was definitely a pronunciation problem in the speech of the disadvantaged pre—schooler. Even after listening to a tape of child and parent speech, which included some aspects of the phonological (and gram— matical) system of the informants, very little comment of a specific nature was added to the data already known of the teacher's concept of the informants' speech. The implications of this data are discussed later in this chapter. Data of this investigation proved, too, that the teacher was correct in her estimate of the parent's speech problem being similar to that of the child. The very young child, such as the pre—schooler in the Head Start program, apparently has not had the time to have had the peer influence on his language in a phonological way. In— stead, the evidence indicates similar delections in the phonological patterns presented in this chapter. 1149 .aHHmoHHHooom COHuonc HchoE\u\ osoom meCmE Ion om on» no oCOC oCm COHpmHmp on psonm wCHCCOC omCOHquE mCono mm m:. . . .CCo CCw mHoCHE .wCHC ICwaonnmCCoz CH whoppmH pHEo mmCB: "mHe Cocomme A.CmuhmmCoCCHx "who: me CH CommawnoCmCmo Ho mom» ConuCOHpmHmo HmHomEn\p\ Ho mHCmemv .mCOHpmHoo 03p ohms mCoCo CoHC3 CH Commaw CHHCo Ho wow» m 0» wCHCmpmHH Cmuum CoCUHHCo 0C» no Comoam on CH \p\ 90 CoHpmHoU HchoE me cmCOHpCmE mHQEwm mCo CH nCoCommp om me Co mcoz Swopmummm CCm CmpmHm ”moCoz mmcCp CH CoomCmICHHCo Ho mom» CounCOHponn HchoE|\p\ Co mmHQmemv umoumoocmhpsooo nomumoocohhsooo pmNHHmom Omudemm Ho ocm no can «Hm Rom Ho COHpmHmQ no COHponQ .mCCoz mCHoCCOCOCQ mH EmHQOCQ Coooam Canoe mCu: mzpmppspw m mH mHCu w mH mHCp mono pmmm om meC mono: m:CCooCoo m mmnmmoCongsooo mmHumooCmuHSOOO mH mpCCOm Ho COHosoHmeCw: mzmHoECE mono: omNHHmmm omNHHmom m:Cooon HNCH omsh: mz. . . CCHm mmCu: ”mm Cosm mewEmC oxms ou CmCu Cono COHo mm H mCOHpmHmQ mm u mCOHponQ :Hmoo HmemE oCo CH \p\ Co COHpoHop me HMHumwoCmCCSooo HmHummoCmphsooo prOC quemm on» CH mCoComoo oCo Ho oCoz HMHqupom HMHqupom CoHpHmoq HmHCmE CH "\p\ ocmcomCoo doom Ho COHuonQ :.woCsom Co mCCoz Hmo wCHouso: mp prmE mono uMCz Co mpcsom OHHHoQO COHpCmE uOC CHC mCo IComop 03» mmon CCw .mocsom Ho COHponU omCOHpCoE mCmComop Cmeo on» Ho mCOC mzmohoz 90 who wCHpuso: on psoow wCHCpHCm onm mCmCommp m omon HHCO :.wUCoz Ho mCCm mCOCCm wmoze: "mmx CoCowme :.m©Coz CHme "mm* CmComoB was CCO wCprso . . . Ho meHpCm wmp HHm Pmo use zone: .CoHpmHme mHCp psoow waEoC 0: come mCmComou um Cono on mCmNHHMCchw 3mm CCw mmu A.C0HpHmoo . umooCoCCCooo ummoCoCCCooo COan CH saw so: eHo mCm puma =.m.u use How emNHHmmm HHN swuHHmmm m.p on ooCCOCOCQwHE HoCB: ”mHm HoCommB ACooQOnuCoCmm Ho mECmp CHV Ho CCm Ho CCm .\o\ HmCHH Co COHomHoc COHoCmE pOC oHo t mCoCommp Cmeo ComoCo>mm 0C9 .om UCm .mm now no» .mH .mH .HH .mH .3H .NH .HH .0H .m mCmpEgC u C0 CoHpmHmo u C0 CoHumHmo "mum: mCoCommo owoCB .waHUCm CCCOm mmo wCH>me: Co :HCO mCHopso: CmCoHHCo omme m mH mHCp m mH mHCp ozone mpCmEopmow HmCmme come mm me Ho Co>on .Hms zcm CH \C\ oCmComCoo doom mCu COHpCoE COC CHC mCmCowoo mm wCHCHwEmC one =.p- ecm .e- .mCH. Csz mHnCOCo m>mC CmCCHHCo ommCB "2mm CoCommB :.mHCCOCo Eme m>Hw w.» oCm m.p mHummoCmnusooo UGNHmem mmummoCmCCCooo womaammm mHHu mCOHpmHmQ :H u mCoHumHoa Apom CCm pCCoumCCoz mmme CH Commam mCo mmmCHpCm CCO o>moH moCB: no} CmCommB HaHummoCmCCsooo mmnmooconnsooo -nHHCo Co mama CoacoHpuHme HmCHC-\u\ co meaemxmv AcommamueHHzo co mega» CHV HmeCmpom HmHquuom COHonom HCOHwOHoCOCQ HmCHH CH ”\o\ onCOwCoo doom mCu Ho COHumHoo Cowoom Coomqm mama HuHHd mCoHuqmohmm m.meomoB one pomoCoo HmeHCH m_CmCommB on» CH m.pcmumm ms» CH m.cHHCo on» CH Ememwm H<0Hooqozomm mmB mo maommm< msom 150 .Commam pCmCmQ Ho oCmQ on Co \C\ Com \C\ pr Ho COHpsp IHpmnsm me meOHpCoE mCoComop mCu Ho oCoz .CoHumep oCu COHpCoE uoc UHU whosommp mm Hmzpo ch .COHumHmv \C\ H0 HHN u ICsooo CmNHHmmm mwocmcm RHH u mooCmC Ipzooo noNHHmmm HmCome CH mxoaw 0mm CmComme .wCHCCOE CH \C\ Ho AComemIpCoCmq Ho wECop CHV Ho Cam Co pCm COHponU me Co comeEmC HHHCOHHHoQO OHH CmComme .EooH HmonoHOCOCQ mHCp COHpCoE COC Has no COHumHoQ «mm Ho coprHmQ A.wCHCCoE UCm wCHuHmH> ”mppoz mmmCu CH Comoam CH0 mCoCommo Cono m>HCI>on39 :.wCH_ co m w m m m m m IpCmeQ Ho mam» me COII\C\ COH \C\ we moHQmemv mCo moCCOCOCQ Cm>m p.Cop zone: "QNC CmCommB H HCp H HCp .COHumHoU mCu COHpCmE no: :.CHo pCmH MHnmmoCompsooo mHnmmoCoCCsooo CHU mCmCommp CmCoo om mmeHCCo Co moCoz CmHsoHp HHHmzm: mCm mpCsom. wCH_ oCH: ”mHm CoComoB UoNHHmmm omNHHmmm ICwQ COHpCoE 90C CHC pan .mcczom Ho wCHQQOCC me :.Cmo m on» wCH>moH Hp .wCH I I “Howme CH .UmCOHpCmE mm on .om .mH .mu mCmCommB IxooH. mm Cosm oCos a 5mm sz9: ”OHm CmComoB m: I mCoHponQ ow I mCOHpmHmQ A.MCHwCHm ocm wCHHHmp "moCoz mmmCo CH Cowoqm :._CHom m.pH .wCHQw Ho omomeH: "om CoCowwe HmummoCmCCzooo HmumooCmCCsooo ICHHCo Ho mom» me C0II\C\ Com \C\ Co meQmemv AComemICHHCo C0 mECmp CHV HmHoCmoom HprCopom \c\ COC \C\ H0 CoHpspHpmnsm New u mmoCmC “so u mmoCoC ICsooo noNHmem Ipsooo pmNHchm Ho oCm Ho oCm .CoHpmHop on» psonm CoxCMEmC mHQEMm Rom amm on CH mm Cme0 me Ho mCoz C. . . . mHCoHE Ho COHumHmQ Ho CoHpmHmQ me . . . CH mCmppoH pHCo Hmcez "mHH CmCommH . A.CmCUHHCo :.mpCmC0mC00 ooCCOCOCQ o_Coo: :.Cme0Cm m mH CHCH m mH mHCo CCm HCHCmoCoz ”moCoz me CH ComemIoCmCmo Ho COH CCsom mCo mpsuHomnCm Hon: mep Co oznmooCmCCsooo mmumooCoCCsooo camp COIICoHpmHmC COHpHmoQ HmHUoEIC\C Ho oHQmemv :szH: :.omHCE:E: :.CmCC:Hm: mH COHC3 own» CmNHHmmm UoNHHmom .\C\ HmHCmE mm acmECOHCH pHsom pCm CHHCo on» Coon Co I I no COHpmHmC on» meoHpCmE mCoComou om m:» we mCoz COHuwHoCCOCOCQ oCo oo CmHoC 0p Coco Cmeo mH I mCoHpmHmQ mH I mCOHpmHmQ A.mCoCCCCC "mono: wHCo CH CooQOICHHCo Ho COHuHmoq HmHCmE on CH \C\Ho COHuoHoC oCo wmnmmoCmmpsooo wmumooCmCCsooo mam» COIICOHpmHoC CoHpHmoa HmHUmEI\C\ Ho mHQmemv mo0C oHQEmm me CH mCmCowop mCo Co mCoz HprCouom HmHoCmoom COHpHmoa HmHUmE CH ”\o\ pCmCowCOC doom oCo Ho COHpmHmQ . HmmnmooCoCCsooo mm.mz n mmoCmC CwNHHmmm ICsooo cmNHHmmm .wCHCHOC CHmm mCmeo .EoHCOCQ mHCp C0 Hm mmpCmEEoo HmCmCow cows 03% :.m.p Ho UCm Ho pCm oCmEEoo HOC CH0 mCmComop mm meOHpmHoC ozone mpCmE Ccm p.o ooCCOCOCowHE Hone: ”HHH CmCommB RoH Hm.om IEoo HmCmme come oH CCw .mH .OH .3 .mm mCoComme HComwomIoCmCmQ Ho mECmp CHV Co COprHmQ Ho COHpmHmQ H.CCm w CCOC mCo CH ComemIpCoCmq Co .CocmEocmCQ m m m m m m mom» COIICOprHmC COHuHmOQ HmCHHI\C\ Ho mHQmemv mHCp osoom wCHCCOC pHmm mCmCommo Cono NH H HC» H HCp .EmHCOCo mHCo Co quEEOo COC CH0 mCoComop mmeHoCm ocm mCCsom cCo CH0 wCHopso Co oxoom omummonCCsooo omummoCoCCsooo m>HCI>pCm39 .mpCoz mo mpCo me EOCH mCmppmH owCowop Co>mHm :.oI Ccm CI .mCH I Csz woNHmem nmNHmem Hue wCprso pCm mCHoQOCo CmCCHHCo Ho EmHCOCQ mHnsogp m>mC CmCCHHCo owmce. "zmx CoCome I I Commom on psoom omNHHMCoCow mmm pCm mm» mCmComoB _ :.m.p UCm m.© am I mCOHpmHmQ mm I mCOHponQ A.CCC I CCm Ime umcpoz mmme CH ComemICHHCo Co oCoIImeHUCm CH0 o>mmH HmCB um* CwCommH mMHnmmoCmCCCooo NOHnmmoCmCCsooo comp COIICOHumHmC COHpHmog HMCHHIU\C Ho moHomemv ACooonIoHHCo Ho mECoo CHV HwHoCmuom HmeCmoom COHpHmoo HCOHonOCOCo HCCHH CH "\C\ CCmCOmCOC Loom me Ho COHpmHmQ 151 .Commom ucmaQ 90 was» on» Co Chaos HonCCsCon CCOCOCQ Ho mmmwo 03» on» COHuCoE poc CHC mCmCommp Hm wCHCHmEmC 0C9 =.HHuomCCooCH Hug Hon» meHCp on» Co sou m pmsw mud omonp ..mCm CopnwsmC HE. . . .= "HHH CmCommB :.pru Hum HHC Hme van ..oCm Cmprsz .uomnnzm oHCCOC me Com: Hone: "NHH Concave =.oou .xwoam CoCCHHCo on» Hm: .mCm Counwsz Hz: ”NH CmCome “.mCm CHHOCmo CCm .mnm CmpcwsmC HE “who: CooQOIpCmaQ mo comp cc» COIIHoCmCCCCmC CCOCOCC on» Co mHQmemv .Coomam m.CHHCo on» Co pomqmm HCUHquEma mHCp Co pCmEEoo 0C oCme mCmCommp Cmeo prHmIHquze :.Cmeo some on uxoC quHC mpoonnsm 03» CH ado Hone: "Hm CmComwB =....mCm mEmE. HmoHaHp mCCCOm pace: "mmH CmCome A.mCm CmCWHm HE CCw .mCm wEEmE HE ”who: CommawICHHCo Ho mom» on» COIIHoCmCCCCmC CCOCOCQ mCu no mmHQmemv HE. «C» meH m.mep . . .CoomamlpCmCmq Ho mECoo CH CH CoHpCoE oo CmoomHmmC maComou on no om HH< .CHHLO mg» Ho Common oCu CH HoCmCCCCmC CCOCOCQ psonm ComeEmC mCmComou mm Cono on» no mCoz . CCOCOCQ HmComCoa m omr HoCE: AComonICHHCo mCu Ho mECmp CHV .Csoc w Cmumm ”CH CmCommB mC com Hz ow HCHw mepHH Hz mCm nonpoE Hz on Coocwsz Hz ma CHHOCmo Hon» mCHx H2 ow szomop mCB "Ho mm: me CH mumC wwwswaH on» no moCoCH>m oCm mEEmE H: on nonposn H: on Homo C30 oCm CmeoE HE mCm Cmpme Hz mCm CHmCoo Hz "Co mm: on» CH wowC owmswCCH on» Co ooCoCH>m HonCCSCom CCOCOCC .HCspm on» CH 0» CoCmpmHH mnonomwp me CoHC3 Commom pConQ on» CH Couczoo .pmon pm .moHawao Coon who: when» poH .Commom quCmo me CH COHpooHC Cm: 0C» CoCOHpCoE owComou om 0C» Co oCo poz H.30Cx mCm Con «owap o» o>0H mCm .CHMCCC p0: mCm .Eme Csz oEoo mCm thmz ComemIpCoCmq Ho mom» on» COIICoHpomHHCH Cm: 0:» Co mmHQmemv .CmCCHHCo Ho EmHnoua Comoam m mm COHuomHH ICH m on» COHpCmE poc CHC mCmCowmp mm Cmeo HH< .EmHCOCQ mCu mm CmEmC HHHwOHHHCQO COC mm: mHCp pan .COHpooHCCH m C0 mm: mCC 0» omuCCHCppw mp CHCOC .mHH CmCommE Hp mCmE oCCp mm Cosm :.moCo> wCHSOCx uOC: on moCoConH w mquCmm :.Coomam wCHCmuHOCm Ho mHmz ow mom oC . . .: "MH CoComme A.mmxHH H I CCm I wCOCz has» umCoz ComemICHHCo Ho mom» on» COIICOHuomHHCH :x: me no moHQmemv .CooonIpCmCma CH COHuomHHCH m me COHuCoE uOC CHC waCommp mm Conuo mcfi ”..wmonp. .mmz. ..mpme. COC .mmmCC_ CCw CHGW LmLHOE WHSB: .mumz. pow "HH CmComoB HCommmmIpCmCma me Ho mEme CHV .Comodm CH CoHuowHHCH m on» COHquE no: CHC mCmComop CmCuo w>HCIHpCm35 :..mmmw. Com :..HHQD mHS mCHNEU. mC m.mep. .mwm. How Hone: Ham CHzos HoCB: Ho CmmprH "0mm HwCome .HHCC ”:HH monomme :.oum nCoo.H COH wCOOH mCmHH Com moCmHH Hmm HoCB: ”CH Accomme CH mm .m pr Hmo m>mmH Hone: CCm> CCHCS Hpr p.Cmo HoCB: HCommomICHHCo me Co mECop CHV :..®®m on. ”0* CmCommB =.mm: on ”2* CmCommH HmstCHm CHM an> .m mHmCCQ CCoz .H "no mm: on CH momC mwmsmcmH Co mocmCH>m wEmom HmCHEOCOHm .m CmstCHm Cum nso> .m meCCHQ Csoz .H “Ho mm: 0:» CH mpr owwsowH .Ho moCoCH>m coHpowHCCH Cm: one mama CopHC mCOHpqooHom m.CmCommB oCB pqwoCoo HmeHCH m.CmCommB on» CH Cowmam m.pCmCmm on» CH Coomqm m.CHHCo on» CH EmBmHm HCoo uoc ago: no who: mquCmn CC» 30; Ho COHquE 0C mCmE mHQEmm 0C» CH maComou mm Cono mCE :.HHmeH wCHmeu mCm Hon» pmme pm Han .Hmm on mCCoz CwCoCo wCHSOCx Cm>w CCm mCCoz wCHoCCOCOCQ oEHp CHCC m w>mn ow wCHCmpmHH mC.o3 mpCmCmn wmeEC "HH CmCommE :.mam» mHCo C0 MCHOC ohm HmCu mm .Hono Como Cqu mew>Coo on prm on on Emwm 0C HoC» usn HmLmCummu on» o» HHnmuHomEoo xmwmm cu CMCOCm mCCos m>mC p.C0C Hose: "0* CoComoB =.Hmm ou HOH m o>mn CCm CmCuo Como CuHs HHmz HCO> wCOHm How 0» Econ .mH vac» .mpCmCmq wC» .HmCa= "MH CoCome ..Hmm on HOH m o>mC on Eoom HwCE: ”NH CoCommE ”mCoComoo on» EOCH memEmC omon CmuHoHHo mom» :0 HCmHCnaoo> m.quCmC on» Co mmHQmemv .mEAop HMCmCmM Co OHHHoon ConHm CH .HCmH Ianwoo> pr o» Couch Co: CHC mCmCommp Hm CmCuo one .HHC um MCHmeu Etna Cum: op om IwwH300C0 Em H . .omH Cocomme =.Ew£p Ham 0» 30C CC“ mCCoz on» soCx ».C0C push HOCu pan CmCOCo HHmmCH wCHme» oCm CoCCHHCo ommCE= .CNH CwCommB :.wCCoHCH CHoCu Co wHou CHme psonm wCHmeu mum: Hon» Cmcz HHwoCH HCw> Comeu HoCEC umHH CoComoB A.mCoComwp 0C» EOCC memeC omen» Co éHoHHm mama Co HCmHsnmoo> m.CHHCo me no mmHQmemv :.mCo3mCm CnozI mco: =.wCCoz mHCEHm: :.HCmH:nmoo> MCmHm: :.mCC03 mHanHHmI mCo own: :.mCC03 on opsmoaxo 0C: :nHCmHsomoo> CommmE: "Ho oxoqm HwCB .mucoumm CHmCu Ccm CwCCHHCo wCu Cuon Co moHCmHznmoo> prHEHH HCm> 0C» 0» CmCCoHoC mCmCommu om mCu Ho HHm HHCme HCw> .CoHumsuHm Hoonom on» CH mHnmuCOCEoo uHoH HmCu Cmcz Co mCCwHCH Co mambo CHme CCH: mCH Ixmmnm mp0: Hme CmC: HwaCH CCm .HHxOsz «HwHHmw exonm muConn oCu no HCCS .HCmHsnmoo> CCWHHow Chm mquhma wEom .HMm 0» oHupHH CmC CCm HCm CCm poHsv HCw> who: mpCopmn oEow .COHumsunIHmHQ m CH Co whoma CHme CCH: mCH Ixmmnw owz HoCu CmCs EmemCCqu CuHs CCm HmeCC mxoam cmCCHHCo oCH Ho Hcmz .mquE Imduh COCooCmm on» Co oEom .HHm um swoon oCu Ho mEom Emamum Hx mmB mo wavmmm< mzom .w>Huoquwa =mCoP. mC» wCHOHCOC CmCOHpCCE mponumou om on» Co mCoz H.CCCC3 oCOC oCm ”mm: CoomeIquCmm Ho can» on» COIIHCCHHHxsm w>HuomuCod CcCOC: on» Co oHnEmev .Coomam ICHHCo mC» CH o>HuomCCwa =mCOC= mCu Co mm: «Cu CmoHCOC oCm can» CmCoHquE mCoComop mCu Ho mco uoz H.pow CCOC H "mm: CoomCmICHHCo no mama MC» COIIHCmHHHxsm c>HuooHCon :mCOC: on Ho onmemv .prCmu mHC C0\CCm CHHCo «Cu Ho Cowmaw CC» mCHCCwoCoo CwCuHm .30H>C0»CH meommu HCm CH pcommnn mm: Homamm HMoHmeEMCw mHCo Ho COHuCoE 02 CH mCOC CHHOCmo pH mCOC oC uoHndem Com HHMHHHCCC pr ”Ho mm: on» CH ame ommsmCmH Ho wocmCH>m oCOC mEsz wCOC on "oHndew Com HumeHxsa CC» "Ho mm: on» CH CHMC wwmswaH Co ooCmCH>m HCmHHHxsm o>HoowHCmC :mCOC. mCB .m>Humwa oHCCOC wCu CwCOHuCoE oHaEmw mCu CH maComou om me Ho mCoz H.CoCCHHCo 0C o>mC u.C0C HmCu .CCd .MCHCHOC 0C u.C0C 03 "who: CoownquCoCma mo cam» on» C0II®>HumwoC mHCCOC mCu no mmHamemv .mCMu C0 CbCCHHCo on» Co Cowmam oCu CH m>HuwwoC wHCCOC oCu Ho mm: on» CoCOHuCoE oHCEmw map CH mCoCommu om me Ho wCoz H.mMCHCp 0C w>mC H.C0C H ”mm: CowwmmICHHCo Co comp CC» COIIw>Humwa wHCCOC 0:» Co oHawamv .quCma wHC C0\CCm CHHCo oCu CmCuHo Co Eopme HCOHumEEwa mCu Ho oomamw Cm mm: m>Hpmme oHCCOC wCu awn» CocoHquE anEmm msu CH mCoCommu om oCu Ho wCoz «mm H woonC Ipsooo CmNHHmmm no CCm HwH Co COHumHeC m mH mHCp mflmwoflmhhfiooo CmNHmeC mm u wCoHpmHmo Nzlmvocwhhflooo Hmecouom um: u moonC ICCooo CoNHHmmm Co CCm «mm «0 COHponQ m mH mHCu mHIMOOEOHRSOOO UONHHmOM mw I mCOHuoHoQ mmummoConuaooo HwHucmuom m>Humwwz mHnsoo on» Co om: 153 The language data reveals that both the pre—school children and their parents do have a language which they use freely when they are in the company of peers or those with whom they are familiar. The teachers, as a group, failed to recognize the fact that the children and adults of the lower socio—economic level group speak a systematic social— dialect rather than the more normative language of the school. It is a language system, nevertheless, that permits communication between children and their parents. An observation of the inter-action of individuals of the lower socio—economic level group will reveal this; this is an important observation that every teacher should strive to make for the implications this knowledge holds for the teaching-learning process. The Language of the Teacher The language behavior of the teacher in this speech study was of a very careful and cautious nature. Because of this, both the phonological and grammatical evidence secured was not evaluated in terms of its meaning to the investigation as was the speech of the pre—schooler and his parent. It will suffice to say that the teacher very care— fully avoids some aspects of the phonological and grammatical systems of the child and his parent, such as: the deletion of final consonants and the substitution of word endings; the "s" inflection, the use of the double negative, the pronoun.redundancy, or the "done" perfective auxiliary——if 154 they ever did exist in the speech of the teacher. According to previous studies in both New York and Detroit (which have been referred to many times in this investiga— tion), there is proof that these aspects of phonology or grammatical usage exist in smaller quantities or not at all in the speech of the middle socio—economic group to which most teachers belong. The biographical data of this study indicate that the teacher is the recipient of more formal academic education and travel opportunities both of which are social indices of language usage. The teacher, therefore, is more apt to be the possessor of a form of standardized English. Finally, her training in the language arts in her preparation to teach the elementary grades has prepared her for her unique awareness of the value of enunciation in the use of the language. In this concluding chapter of the language study, the attempt was made to Specifically identify and describe some aspects of the language behavior of the disadvantaged pre—schooler and their parents and the teachers' concepts of this language, both initially and after listening to a tape of child-parent speech. The question now arises: After the identification and description of the language have been made, what can the teacher do about the gap which exists between the English of the pre~schooler from the dis— advantaged home and the more standardized language of the school and the society in which the child will seek to live and to work in eventually? 155 Implications of this Language Study The typical elementary language arts classroom throughout the nation today uses the integrated language arts approach to the teaching of the skills and apprecia— tions of the language, but does not concern itself with just hdw we communicate and hdw language works. At the pre-school and primary level there is some emphasis on the oral approach to language, but not nearly enough. In the work of Kohl, it was discovered that the disadvantaged child, in particular, SEE talk and write freely, but often determines not to because of the hostile reception he rears.l Traditionally, too, the teacher's approach to the teaching of the language arts is a negative or corrective one. In such an atmosphere, the child from the disadvantaged home, which does not usually possess the use of the norma— tive school language, does not try to communicate with the teacher. The implications of this study are evident, as seen in the language data of the investigation and the teachers' initial and current perceptions of the language problem: 1. re—education of the teacher is imperative, and 2. curriculum revision essential. The teacher most certainly should learn more about the nature of language and the particular needs of the dis— advantaged child. 1Herbert T. Kohl, Teaching the 'Unteachable' (New York: New York Review, 1967). 156 Even though the teacher was asked to specifically identify the language problems of the disadvantaged child, most of them failed to do so. The majority imagined that the child could not or would not communicate because he lacked "words." A few others suggested, however, that they, the children and parents, did speak freely with each other-- though they lacked adequate vocabulary. What the teacher failed to see was that the child and parent did communicate with and without the use of words, but that their means of communication was simply not understood by the school staff. The teachers spoke a different level of language and did not recognize the language system of the lower socio-economic level speaker. As evidence of the need for in-service re—education of teachers, the statements of the teachers themselves should be considered. At the conclusion of the second interview with the teachers, they were asked to give their opinions of the language behavior of the disadvantaged children whom they were teaching. It will be noted that many of these expressions of concern are moralistic judg— ments on people rather than on the language. Many teachers are misinformed. They evidence the insecurity of the pedagogical stereotype. The follOwing statements from the teachers are powerful arguments for the need for more in— service education ofteachers in the fields of language and the psychological and social needs of the disadvantaged child. Teachers #1: #2: #3: #4: #5: #6: #7: #8: #9: #10: #11: #12: #13: They don't talk much at all. They slur words together. Their trouble is just lazy speech. These people cannot distinguish between sounds. Pronounciation is the main speech problem they have. Their problems are communication and expression. Vocabulary is the biggest problem. The problem is just simple com— munication. Just getting them to talk is the biggest challenge. They mumble their words. Their background is the biggest trouble. The substitution of sounds is a concern. Dialect is the problem with these children. 157 Reactions of the Investigator An empty jargon-like statement which is disproved by the studies of Kohl. Naturally they do; this is the way language works. This is a moral judgment. This is a very broad statement. If it means that the disadvantaged speaker cannot distinguish "pig" from "big," this is a nonsense. This is much too broad to be meaningful and is probably wrong. This is not true, unless communica— tion is defined as communication with the teacher. This is probably the smallest problem. Once again, as stated in reaction to #6, this is not true. The greater problem for the teacher is getting children to learn. This may be a problem of enuncia— tion which the teacher may teach or a hearing problem on the part of the listeners. The statement is too vague to be of use. Unless the sound substitutions can be identified and described, this statement is meaningless. We all have a dialect. Teachers #1“: #15: #16: #17: #18: #19: #20: #21: #22: #23: #2H: Just to get children to converse with each other is hard. They talk so fast they stutter. The major speech problem is pro— nouncing words. The biggest problem is that the child learned to talk from hearing his parents talk. The major problem besides their limited vocabulary is the dropping of endings. They omit sounds at the beginning; they substitute. Their pronouncia— tion is bad. They point at things rather than say words. Their main ways of communicating are by movements of the arms and other gestures. The biggest problem is the grammar of the language. The misuse of the pronouns is the most obvious trouble. 158 Reactions of the Investigator This is entirely wrong, as can be proved if the listener will observe the disadvantaged at play or in a relaxed and familiar setting. A remark such as this is psycho— logically erroneous and linguistically naive. Once more, this statement is false. We all do; how else? What is meant by this? The endings of what? Which ones? Are all of them dropped? This probably means morphemes, such as re—, de—, and the like. By whose standard is the pronounciation bad? This is a cultural bias. Don't we all use gestures to communicate an idea at times? If we would feel intimidated, as the disadvantaged often do, we would do the same. Much too broad a statement to have any real meaning or to be of much use. Does this teacher mean they for I? W3 for ydd? This is probably not the meaning intended, but the teacher is still not descriptive enough. Teachers #25: #26: #27: #28: #29: #30: The hardest thing is to express their thoughts in com- plete sentences. A limited vocabau— lary is the biggest problem. They need to know more and more words. They Speak so quick- ly they often stutter. They mumble their woods. Their speech is just very careless and lazy. 159 Reactions of the Investigator Not to be philosophical, but what's a thought? What's complete? What's a sentence? Certainly not the biggest. This is not necessarily so. As was stated in the reaction to #15, this statement is erroneous and naive. As was stated in the reaction to #10, this staement may be a problem of hearing. This is a moralistic judgment. This list suggests to the investigator tremendously naive attitudes toward language and lack of understanding of the disadvantaged. cation of teachers in: It reveals a need for workshop edu- 1. Cultural relativity--learning to respect different social systems; culturals, learning to reSpect other other racial and ethnic groups—-without forming moral judgments of different peoples. 2. The nature of language——learning to describe language problems first in order to do something about them later (for an illustration of this point see statement of Teacher #18); learning what kinds of problems are most crucial (for an illustration of 160 this point see statement of Teacher #7). Teachers should learn to describe language in terms of: (a) Phonetics; (b) Grammar; (c) Syntax. Suggestions for Implementation of Language Data Findings in Terms of the Classroom There is a contrast between the language data discover— ies of this investigation and those of the Detroit Dialect Study which suggest a type of age—oriented pedagogical implication. 1. What differences in age-grading suggest differences in language arts sequencing? The use of the multiple negative suggests that the five year old pre-schooler from the disadvantaged home uses the double negative 58% of the time; the ten year old, according to the Detroit Dialect Study, uses the multiple negative 55% of the time; this is only a 3% difference yet the ten year old has five more years of academic experience than the five year old pre-schooler. This calls for a concentrated effort on the part of the teacher to teach this aspect of the child's grammatical system with more meaning and more consistency and at an earlier stage in the child's academic experience. 2. How much interference can we expect from parents speech? The pre-schooler speaks with much the same phonological and grammatical aspects of language behavior as does his parent: In the case of the multiple negative, the parents 161 of the pre—schooler from the disadvantaged home used this- grammatical index 78% of the time; in the investigation of the Detroit Dialect Study, the parents of the older children used the multiple negative 74.6% of the time. In both studies indications are that there is much interference from parent speech in the speech of children 5 to 12 years of age. In the studies of Labov this is diminishing percentage depending on the age and academic skills of the child. 3. What different phonological and grammatical forms can be expected from disadvantaged children? Although this was not an exhaustive study of the phonological and grammatical systems of the disadvantaged child, the language data of the investigation revealed some aspects of the language behavior of the child from the lower socio—economic level group in Detroit: a. Some expectations which can be anticipated in the phonological system of the disadvantages pre— schooler: (l) deletion of the stOp consonant /t/ in the final phonological position (79% of the time). (2) deletion of the stop consonant /t/ in the medial position (20% of the time). (3) deletion of the stOp consonant /d/ in the final phonological position (50.8% of the time). It is further suggested that in this aspect of phenology, the /d/ stop may be substituted for the /t/ stop in the final position. However, whether /d/ is realized as /t/ or /d/ phone— mically, as in "walked," it is deleted by the disadvantaged: 30% before vowels 36% before consonants, and substituted for 3% before vowels and 9% before consonants. A. 162 (A) deletion of stop consonant /d/ in the medial position (33% of the time). (5) substitution of /n/ for /n/ (83% of the time). Some expectations which can be anticipated in the grammatical system of the disadvantaged pre— schooler: (l) the "s" inflection: in the noun plurals (such as, no foods, fishes); and in the verb 3rd singular (such as, she say it, he see it); and in pronominal forms (such as, his self, my's cousins). (2) the pronoun redundancy: such as in my cousin she; our baby he (3) use of the double negative: such as in we don't have no more; I don't have no skates; that ain't no dog (3) the "done" perfective auziliary: such as in my brother done gone; Mamma done gone to the barbecue. How should these forms of phonological and grammatical behavior be treated in the classroom? a. As item drills: The conventional approach to the teaching of language skills is the item drill in which each phonological and grammatical item is taught in isolation and drilled on consistently and periodically. There is a question as to the value of this method of language teaching in terms of its transfer to practical and meaningful use in other contexts. As pattern practice drills: This method advocates the teaching of language usage and phonological learning by presenting the language patterns in 163 oral practice in the classroom. There is no identification or description of the language proceeding this practice and the transfer of this language behavior has not yet been proved in research for the pre—schooler or primary child. The pattern practice drill has been proved to be legitimate language teaching when working with youth of high school age who are motivated to learn the use of the normative school language when school success and preparation for college and work Opportunities are part of the intrinsic motivation. c. As switching devices:2 This is the method of teaching the prestige dialect in such a way that the child learns that there are linguistic choices that each individual must make for himself and that this choice depends upon the circumstances in which the person finds himself. Identification and the description of the language preceeds this method of language instruction and is, therefore, the method this investigation suggests in the teaching of language behavior to the disadvantaged. Roger W. Shuy, director of the Detroit Dialect 2Switching devices are the rules by which a speaker of one social dialect can convert to another social dialect. Such rules have been illustrated earlier (see Chapter V, for example, /d/+/t/ (/d/ realized as /t/). These rules are also reversible so a person whose realization is /t/ might convert to /d/. (Statement by Roger W. Shuy, personal interview, July, 1967.) 16A Study, suggests that locating the switching devices of oral language is no simple task: Our task is not to erradicate the social dialects which are inappropriate in the classroom. On one hand it is uneconomical of our time to approach our job as a classroom manifestations of the Al Capone syndrome; on the other hand it is dangerous to deprive our students of a channel (perhaps the only channel) of communication with people with whom they live. Perhaps no other profession has spent as much time on negatives (spelling demons, jargon, triteness and seven deadly grammatical sins) and as little time on positives (alternate styles, alternate appropriate social dialects) than the profes— sion_owanglish. It has seldom occurred to English teachers that their customers may want or need to switch from schoolroom English to playground English as well as from playground to schoolroom. The switching devices may be more apprOpriate identifiers of the substance of our teaching. The ultimate choice of when to use these switching devices and when not to use them will have to be made by the speaker. We can't legislate virtue, no matter how we define it. But we can, and must, provide the linguistic alternatives. What linguistic differences in racial or ethnic groups should be treated in the classroom? Involved here is the issue of whether Negroes should be encouraged to talk like whites or the Polish should talk like other white Detroiters, for example. This investigation included in its sample only repre— sentatives of the Negro and Caucasian groups, but the Negro children were greatly in the majority; there were 141 Negro children and 15 Caucasians. Regardless of the numbers, how— ever, the issue here is whether or not children should be taught a prestige dialect which is different from that of their disadvantaged home. All groups should be allowed to 3Roger W. Shuy, Locating the Switching Devices of Oral Lan ua e (Sacramento, California: NCTE Institute, April 19, 19575, pp . 12-13. 165 retain their cultural heritage with the knowledge and use of the prestige dialect which will help them to be adequately functioning individuals in the society in which they must live. As suggested in our discussion of locating switching devices, all school children should be provided the skills which will help them to do this, if and when they so choose. Recommendations of this Language Study The recommendations of this study must consider the first implementer of curriculum change and development, the classroom teacher herself. The teacher must become positive rather than negative in her approach to the teaching of language behavior. Through the data discovered in our investigation, it was found that the teacher approached the language problem in the Head Start program with sympathy and resignation, but little outward hope of teaching success in this area. Knowledge is confidence and only with a knowledge of language and children can the teacher hope to be success— ful in her instructional efforts. Pre—service Education of Teachers This investigation of language behavior strongly recommends a closer working relationship be established between the public schools and teacher education institutions. In particular, special preparation should be provided for those students who will teach the disadvantaged. Experiences should be provided those students who come from a culture of the middle—class level but who will be expected to 166 identify with the deprived child. It would seem to me a valuable thing for teachers to face the kind of dys-functionalities and ambiguities and problems in teaching children who come from homes where parents are.missing, children who arrive in school from children's shelters, children who do not fit into our classical lockstep in the public school; in working with families where answers are not so simple and clean and direct or with families who may not know of Harvard or the best school in the West or any of the other kinds of status symbols we all live with. It seems to me that involving a person deeply in this for a long period of time is an absolute prerequisite if teacher education is going to move in the direction of genuinely attempting to help people through the classroom situation. Fortunately, for children of the nation, more and more of the teacher'educationinstitutions now require language courses for the elementary teacher in-training. A few of the institutions provide courses in the history and the nature of language, phonetics, and lexicography. With the growth of such courses, the elementary teacher, always con— sidered a generalist in the field of language, would be better prepared to understand and work with the child who needs training in the use of his language; the disadvantaged child needs this language skill in particular. lgrservice Education of Teachers The most difficult and unpredictable element of cur— riculum change or revision is that which involves the human being. In a large urban school system of over 10,000 teachers, such as is the Detroit Public School System, teacher ¥ “Vernon Haubrich, Conference Report: Remaking the World of the Career Teacher (Washington, D. C.: National Education Association, 1966), p. A9. 167 acceptance is essential, but difficult to achieve because of size if nothing else. Among a group of professionals of this number there are those who are not interested in altering or unwilling to change the habits of their teaching— lifetimes, whether that experience means five years, twenty or more. These individuals must be convinced that the curriculum change is needed, that it is a move in the right direction and that the development is both practical and workable in the classroom. The in-service education programs for teachers in the Detroit Public Schools are continually conducted for the instructors by means of workshops. These workshops are conducted during the school day when released time is pro— vided, after-school, Saturdays and summers. This investiga— tion suggests that a number of different specialists be invited to conduct these workshOps or serve as resource persons to individuals who direct these sessions. These specialists should include anthropologists, sociologists, psychologists, and linguists in particular. The competence which these other disciplines provide would give the teachers the knowledge and understanding they need to work with the disadvantaged children. Educators on the staffs of colleges and universities should be invited to sit down with the teachers to discuss particular problems of learning, and methods of teaching the skills and literary appreciations which are such an integral part of the education of the elementary teacher. 168 Additionally, curriculum guides and bulletins can provide teachers with the materials they need to teach language skills and appreciations. These materials should include lesson plans for the teachers, the theory and practical application of the area under study and other instructional suggestions to help the in—service education program for teachers. The curriculum materials, used with the commercially produced aids, can be the vehicles whereby the teacher adds to her knowledge of both the nature of language and the necessary knowledge of children with particular needs: Increased effort should be made to provide teachers, and special assistants for teachers, with background and the preparation to equip them more adequately with the skills and understandings to work with children in the disadvantaged areas.5 5"A Report of the Investigation: Detroit, Michigan," A Study of Barriers to Equal Educational Opportunity in a Large City (Washington, D. C.: National Education Associa— tion, 1967). BIBLIOGRAPHY 169 BIBLIOGRAPHY Books Bloom, Benjamin 8., Allison Davis, and Robert Hess. Compensatory Education for Cultural Deprivation. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Gleason, H. A. Introduction to Descriptive Linguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1955. Kohl, Herbert R. Teaching the 'Unteachable'. New York: New York Review Book, 1967. Kurath, Hans. Handbook of the Linguistic Geography of New England. Providence, Rhode Island: American Council of Learned Societies, 1939. Labov, William. The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington, D. C.: Center for Applied Linguistics, 1966. Loretan,.Joseph O. and Shelley Umans. Teaching the Dis- advantaged. New York: Teachers College Press, Columbia University, 1966. Mueller, John H. and Karl F. Schuessler. Statistical Reasoning in Sociology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1961. Passow, A. Harry (ed.). Education in Depressed Areas. New York: Teachers College Press, Columbia University, 1963. Smith, B. Othanel, William 0. Stanley, and J. Harlan Shores. Fundamentals of Curriculum Development. New York: World Book Company, 1950. Snedecor, G. W. Statistical Methods. Fifth edition. Ames, Iowa: Iowa State University Press, 1956. Templin, Mildred C. Certain Language Skills in Children: Their Development and Interrelationships. Minneapolis, Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press, 1957. Wylie, Joanne. A Creative Guide for Preschool Teachers. Racine, Wisconsin: Western Publishing Educational Services, 1965. 170 171 Articles and Periodicals Brooks, Charlotte K. "Some Approaches to Teaching English as a Second Language," Elementary English, November, 1964, pp. 728—733. Crosby, Muriel. "English: New Dimensions and New Demands," Dimensions of Dialest, Eldonna L. Evertts (ed.). Champaign, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English, 1967, pp. 1—6. Dailey, John T., Clinton A. eyman, Jr., Dean L. Des Roches. "Research on Language Facility and Dialect Trans— formation in Children," Current Social Dialect Research at American Higher Institutions. Washington, D. C.: Center for Applied Linguistics, April 11, 1966. Dawkins, John. "Linguistics in the Elementary Grades," Elementary English, November, 1965, pp. 762—768. Davis, Allison. "Teaching Language and Reading to Disadvan- taged Negro Children,” Elementary English, November, 1965, pp. 791—797. Eels, Kenneth, Allison Davis, R. J. Havighurst, Virgil E. Herrick and Ralph Tyler. ”The Effects of Environment on Oral Language Development : I," Intelligence and Cultural Differences, quoted by Frank B. May in Elementary English, October, 1966, pp. 587—595. Ferguson, Charles, "Teaching Standard Languages to Dialect Speakers," Social Dialects and Language Learning, Roger W. Shuy (ed.). Champaign, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English, 196A, pp. 112—117. Goodman, Kenneth S. "Dialect Barriers to Reading Compre— hension," Elementary English, December, 1965, pp. 853—860. Griffith, Albert J. "Linguistics: A Revolution in Retrospect," Elementary English, May, 1966, pp. 504— 508. Labov, William. "Stages in the Acquisition of Standard English," Social Dialects and Language Learning, Roger W. Shuy (ed.). Champaign, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English, 196“, pp. 77—10A. Lefevre, Carl A. "Language and Self: Fulfillment or Trauma?" Part II, Elementary English, March, 1966, pp. 230—234. 172 McDavid, Raven I., Jr. "The Cultural Matrix of American English," Elementary English, January, 1965, pp. 13— 21. McDavid, Raven I., Jr. "Social Dialects: Cause or Symptom of Social Maladjustment," Social Dialects and Language Learning, Roger W. Shuy (ed.). Champaign, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English, 1964, pp. 3-9. Mukerji, Rose and Helen F. Robison. "A Head Start in Language," Elementary English, May, 1966, pp. 460—463. Stewart, William A. "Urban Negro Speech: Sociolinguistic Factors Affecting English Teaching," Social Dialects and Language Learning, Roger W. Shuy (ed.). Champaign, Illinois: National Council of Teachers of English, 196A, pp. 10—19. Strang, Ruth and Mary Elas Hooker. "First—Grade Children's Language Patterns," Elementary English, January, 1965, pp. 38-41. Strickland, Ruth. The Language of Elementary School Children: Its Relationship to the Language of Reading Textbooks and the Quality of Reading of Selected Children. Bulletin No. 38, No. A. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University, July, 1962. Wachner, Clarence W. "Detroit Great Cities School Improve— ment Program in Language Arts," Elementary School, November, 1964, pp. 73A—7A2. Waetjin, Walter. "Factors Influencing Learning," South Carolina Guidance News, October, 1962, pp. Government Publications College and University Reports. Public Law 89—10, 89th Cong., H.R. 2362. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Govern- ment Printing Office, April 11, 1965. Office of Economic Opportunity. Parents Are Needed, Project: Head Start. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1965. Office of Economic Opportunity. Points for Parents, Project: Head Start. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1965. 173 Unpublished Material Enzmann, Arthur M. "Final Report: Project Heat Start." Detroit, Michigan: Detroit Public Schools, 1965. McDavid, Raven I., Jr. "The Linguistic Atlas Project." University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, August 1950. Riley, William. "Phonological Indices of Social Stratifi— cation." Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1967. Rickard, David. "Sociological Correlation of the Allophonic Variations of Aleveolar Stops in the Speech of High— land Park School Children." Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1967. Shuy, Roger W. "Progress Report of the Detroit Dialect Study." Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1967. Shuy, Roger W. "Locating the Switching Devices of Oral Language." Speech delivered at the National Council of Teachers of English Spring Institute, Sacramento, California, April 19, 1967. Thomas, Dominic R. "Oral Language Sentence Structure and Vocabulary of Kindergarten Children Living in Low Socio—Economic Urban Areas." Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan, 1962. Wilson, George P. ”Instructions to Collectors of Dialect." Prepared by the American Dialect Society, Greensboro, North Carolina, April, 19AM. Wolfram, Walter A. "Multiple Negation and Social Stratifi— cation," from the report of the Detroit Dialect Study, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1967. Newspapers McDavid, Raven”I., Jr. "A Man Who Says 'Hisn' Labels Hisself Most Dumbest," Detroit Free Press, June 19, 1967. APPENDI CES 17A APPENDIX A MAP OF THE CITY OF DETROIT INDICATING SELECTED SCHOOLS INCLUDED IN THE SAMPLE 176 / 90 , , o... A O O I} v. Q C. o C O O \I\ 1.. «0 wow? I. 3—03—0’2 o APPENDIX B THE EIGHT PICTURES INCLUDED IN THE FIRST INTERVIEWS TO ELICIT THE LANGUAGE OF THE INFORMANTS ,// iol Dov 182 183 KIHy APPENDIX C LETTER TO PRINCIPALS WHOSE TEACHERS WERE INCLUDED AMONG THE SPEECH INFORMANTS 186 187 Michigan State University 701 Cherry Lane, Apt. 101 East Lansing, Mich. 48823 January, 1967 Dear , Principal School During the summer of 1965 and 1966, studies took place through the courtesy and interest of the Detroit Public Schools. These studies attempted to identify and describe the speech of children and adults in Detroit for possible future in-service training and curriculum revision for teachers. At the present time I am on leave from my responsibili— ties as elementary language arts supervisor for the Detroit Public Schools and am working at Michigan State University. It is my purpose to complete the analysis of the 1965 studies which taped the speech of pre-schoolers, their parents, and their teachers in the Head Start program. With your kind permission, I would like to speak with the teacher or teachers on your staff whose names are listed below: My discussion with the teacher will again be taped and will be concerned with the teacher's reactions to and opinions of Detroit speech. The teacher will be asked to respond to the following questions: 1. What do you think are the major problems your children have with vocabulary, grammar, and pronunciation? 2. In what ways does the language of the parents influence the children? What problems with vocabulary, grammar, and pronunciation in the language of the parents are reflected in the problems of the child? The teacher will then be asked to listen and react to a five minute tape of the speech of children who were pre—schoolers in the Head Start program. 188 I shall contact you by phone in the near future so that an appointment can be arranged for me to meet with the teacher, at the convenience of both the school and the teacher or teachers involved. Cordially yours, Anne E. Hughes APPENDIX D STATEMENT TO TEACHERS WHO SERVED AS INFORMANTS FOR THE SECOND INTERVIEWS 189 190 PROJECT: HEAD START Teachers will be asked to react to the following questions: 1. What do you think are the major problems your children have with vocabulary, grammar and pronunciation? 2. In what ways does the language of the parents influence children in your class? What problems with vocabulary, grammar and pronunciation in the language of the parent are reflected in the problems of the child? The teacher will then be asked to listen and react to a five minute tape of the speech of children who were pre-schoolers in the Head Start program. After the teacher's reactions to these questions are taped, the teacher will be asked to respond to these questions: 1. Would you give me your opinion of the Head Start program as you experienced it? 2. What kinds of toys or materials did you use in the program? 3. Did the children have anything to eat during the session in school? A. Would you name other foods the children might have had for breakfast at home, before they came to school for Head Start? 5. We'd like to improve our use of these cards. Can you tell me how the children responded to them? Simply tell me in your own words what the children would have said in describing the pictures, using nouns and verbs particularly, please. APPENDIX E MAP OF UNITED STATES INDICATING THE BIRTHPLACES OF THE MAJORITY OF PARENT— INFORMANTS 191 192 Com Hmm .oo .oHC Roo.ooH proe Ham. . . . . m: ooncso CHoo HH CCCH mmmq . . . . . . moomHQCoCHm mwm mm m2 Mo HmUGHwEwm ng Hoooe HmH . . . . . HCCHmmHmmHC Com . . . . . . . meHomm &mm . . . . . . . mEQOH¢ ”moCmEHOHCHIHCmHmm Ho moomHQCoHHm fill / APPENDIX E TRANSCRIPT OF CHILD AND PARENT SPEECH TO WHICH THE SECOND—INTERVIEW TEACHERS REACTED I: C(l): C(l): C(2): C(2): 0(3): 0(3): 0(3): C(1): C(2): C(2): C(1): Interviewer; C(1), C(2), etc., SPEECH OF CHILDREN AND THEIR PARENTS HEAD START Program: Detroit Public Schools Child; P(l), P(2), etc., Parent Come over here, children. You just came in, didn't you? How did you get to school, Kenny? Walked. Your sister went with the other children to the picnic. How did she get to the bus? My mamma she tooked her. Are you making a cake, Carolyn? Yah. How do you make a cake anyway? Tell me. You take that there thing and you put the stuff in here — and then you — then you — put some more stuff in there. That sounds good, Carolyn. Why are you playing, Honey? I don't have no things to play with there. What would you like to play with? A bike. Do you have a bike at home? No. I done got me a bike. My sister she got a bike. I‘m gonna get me one. Where did you go yesterday, children? To church. What do you do in church? Anyone of you tell me. Singing and telling stories. 19A 195 I: Don't you pray to God, too, children? C(2): Sometimes. C(3): I like God. I: Can anyone of you count to ten? C(1): I can. C(3): Me too. C(2): I can count to ten hundreds. I: Let's hear you. Carolyn, you go first. C(2): One - two — three — four — five - six — seven — eight — nine — ten hundreds. C(1): That wrong. I: Let's hear you tell me all the colors you know, Kenny, can you? C(1): Red — green — blue — yellow. I: What's your favorite color, Madeline? C(3): Red. 1: Why is that your favorite color? C(3): 'Cause it's pretty. I: Children, what are your favorite toys to play with here at school? C(3): Dolls and doll houses. C(1): Trucks and cars and sometimes blocks. C(2): I likes dolls and doll buggy. I: Does your child come to school alone, Mrs. ? P(l): I feel that Kenny — ah - could come alone but he sometimes walks with the other kids. I: What about your child, Mrs. ? P(2): P(l): P(2): P(2): P(l): P(2): P(2): P(l): P(2): 196 Well, my daughter she — well — I have other kids in school and she come with them every morning. But she — I know — could come alone 'cause she Visited the school quite often before. She not afraid of anyone and she done wanna cross the street and everything, you know. Do the children enjoy Head Start, would you say? I think he enjoys it. They talk about it all day when they come home from school. They even play school after they get home. I think Carolyn she really loves it. Even though I think they should not call this school for the deprived. My child she's not deprived. But — ah — some of the children are on our block. Do you think that all mothers feel this way about the program. No, some send their kids just to get rid of them - get them out of the house. I enjoy my kids. I don't want to get rid of them. Sometimes I go running around with them. Some days we don't do nothing but go visiting. This project is a good thing - a really wonderful thing for 'em - you know — . During the days before - noon after noon, you know — they always take a nap — but now I know where they are. They're at school at least. What exactly do you think this program does for your children? I think some children are not quite ready for kindergarten. My son wasn't and — ah — the ones that are are shy or can't get along with other children. They don't have no kids their own age at home. I think they learn more in the Kindergarten and the fifth grades than any other. Do you find that the children like the books at all? Do they talk about stories at home after school? Well, Carolyn she does like stories. If the teacher read any story in school she-talks about it all the day. And she love to talk about it and I talk along with her about it — and — ah — I get to know the story. P(2): P(2): P(l): P(l): P(2): P(2): 197 Does she ever talk about reading a story? She loves reading books and going to the library and everything. She tries to hide from me 'cause she talks to the pictures — and she really thinks that reading, you know. Do you ever go to the library with her, Mrs. ? Oh, yes. I really enjoy the library part. I read some of the books and then Carolyn she tells me the story. I always take my boy to the library. He gets a lot of books sometimes. Do you ever bring home a book for yourself, Mrs. ? No, but I help Kenny when he wants to read a story. We always bring all the books home for the kids. I don't have time to read what with the kids and the house to keep. Do you remember any of their favorite stories? Well now, this morning, you know. I get him ready for school - my big boy and his sister. Carolyn she say she read a story about three bears that come from the library. She was showing me the mamma bear, father bear and baby bear. She was showing me the mamma bear — no, how did she do it? Oh — how the little girl ate the baby bear's porridge. So she ask me what's that? I explain it's like cereal, so then she know. IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII llUllllllllllIIIIHIAllIIIIWIlillllllliWHIIIHIIIIIIH