1mm»wwwmnwnmn 321 301066 5036 1 Bi sue—a». A flingsg L - - -..u- ‘a': '.""”..'. 9 5:" ‘W‘, L'4—uc‘u-Ae_ 'LFCWa Q‘Hl‘kf ‘b'v' 7 _O . -901”. 1, -' --- J W-Iwa't. This is to certify that the dissertation entitled CHILDREN FROM INTACT AND DIVORCED FAMILIES: PERCEPTIONS OF PARENT BEHAVIOR AND LOCUS OF CONTROL presented by Rowena Heather Krakauer has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein PSyChOIOQy Kiwi flak Major professor Ellen A. Strommen Date 5/8/85 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 MSU LIBRARIES v wag? ‘ RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. CHILDREN FROM INTACT AND DIVORCED FAMILIES: PERCEPTIONS OF PARENT BEHAVIOR AND LOCUS OF CONTROL BY Rowena Heather Krakauer A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1985 Copyright by ROWENA HEATHER KRAKAUER 1985 ABSTRACT CHILDREN FROM INTACT AND DIVORCED FAMILIES: PERCEPTIONS OF PARENT BEHAVIOR AND LOCUS OF CONTROL BY Rowena Heather Krakauer This study examined the effects of divorce on later latency children from a nonclinical population. Perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control were assessed for 68 middle class children (34 boys and 34 girls) aged 9 to 12 years selected from 150 volunteer families from a total solicited sample of about 1000 families. Forty children from intact families and 28 children from divorced families completed the Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory (CRPBI) and the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale. Demographic data were obtained from parents for each family. Families which volunteered to participate constituted only 15 percent of those contacted. The representativeness of these families must be questioned, a problem common to most divorce studies working with volunteer families. Results showed a general absence of differences between children from divorced and intact families in their perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control. There were no significant differences between children from divorced and intact families on the three factors of the Rowena Heather Krakauer CRPBI (Acceptance - Rejection, Psychological Control - Psychological Autonomy, and Firm Control - Lax Control), 17 of 18 scales of the CRPBI, and the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale. However, the Possessiveness scale of the CRPBI indicated that children of divorce perceived both their mothers and fathers as significantly more possessive than did children from intact families. Children saw their mothers as significantly more psychologically controlling than their fathers. Within the divorced sample, several indices suggest that children viewed parents as more rejecting with increasing age. Father's remarriage, the years since father's remarriage, and custody arrangement were the divorce variables that affected children's perceptions the most. The results showed that parental divorce did not impact strongly on children's perceptions of parents and locus of control and suggest minimal adverse effects of divorce, at least for children from the volunteer nonclinical population studied. Cautions that must be exercised in attempting to generalize the results from this study, in common with previous work, are discussed. Implications for future research and clinical interventions are also discussed. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I want to thank the members of my dissertation committee: Dr. Ellen Strommen, Chairperson, Dr. Gilbert DeRath, Dr. John Hurley, and Dr. John Hunter. I am especially grateful to Dr. Strommen for assuming the leadership of this committee; she provided me with support and expert advice throughout my doctoral studies and research. A resounding thanks goes to Dr. Bruce Tabashnik for his steadfast support and scientific contributions to every phase of this project. Bruce's patience, enthusiasm, and loving partnership inspired me as I conducted my dissertation research. I would like to thank Dr. Lucy Rau Ferguson for her fine teaching, support, and guidance. Dr. Ferguson greatly influenced my development as a family therapist and researcher. A number of people facilitated the recruitment of subjects. I wish to thank the following individuals for their cooperation: Dr. Jeremy Hughes, Superintendent of the Haslett Public Schools, James Pocock, Ingham County Friend of the Court, Toni McFarland, Karen Glickman, Erni Goldstein, Joey Letterman, Thea Glicksman, Dr. Lawrence iii Schiamberg, William Rosenthal, and Barbara Scott. Maurice Moses and Scott Bennett were dedicated research assistants. I am extremely grateful to the children and parents who volunteered to participate in this study. Their participation made this research possible! I appreciate the helpful comments and friendship of Elizabeth DeRath, Fred Rogosch, Jane Pearson, Nick Ialongo, Laurie Jayne and Randy Pepper, and Vicki and Duane Silverthorn. Suzy Pavick was always available with information and assistance during my graduate program. She deserves a special thanks. I am especially grateful to my parents, Dr. Bernard and Edith Krakauer, for their constant love and their generous support of my education. I thank my grandmother, Sylvia Schacter; my brother, Dr. Jesse Krakauer; and my sisters, Sabrina Krakauer and Melissa Krakauer for their encouragement and support. I also want to thank Phyliss and David Friedman, Dr. Ines Krakauer, Leah Tabashnik, Susan Tabashnik, and David Tabashnik for cheering me on during this study. I was supported in part by a National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Training Fellowship, a Graduate School Scholarship, and by the Department of Psychology. I thank Dr. Gordon Wood and Roger Halley for facilitating financial assistance and research reimbursements. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES O O O O O O O 0 INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . STATEMENT OF PURPOSE . . . . . REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . Background on Children of Later Latency Children of Children's Perceptions of and Locus of Control . Divorce . . . . Divorce . . . . Parent Behavior Children of Divorced and Intact Families from a Normal Population . . . . . . . HYPOTHESES O O O O O C O O O 0 METHOD 0 O O O O O O O O O O 0 Design . . Subjects . Procedure . Instruments RESULTS 0 I O O I I O O O O 0 Comparisons between Children from Divorced and Intact Families . . . Comparisons between Mother and Father Effects of Age . . . . . Effects within the Divorced Group . . Effects of Demographic Variables on Children's Perceptions of Mother . Effects of Demographic Variables on Children's Perceptions of Father . Effects of Demographic Variables on Locus of Control . Comparisons between the Custodial and Non-Custodial Parents . . . . . V mum-.5 11 14 16 16 16 21 22 29 DISCUSSION 0 O I O O O I O O I O O O O O O O 0 Comparisons between Children from Divorced and Intact Families . . . Comparisons between Mother and Father Sex Differences and Effects of Age . Effects within the Divorced Group . . Implications for Future Research and Clinical Interventions . . . . . . APPENDICES O O I O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O A. Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory (CRPBI) and scoring sheet . . B. Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale C. Letter to Parents . . . . . . . . . . . D. Parent Demographic Survey Form . . . . REFERENCES 0 O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 vi Page . 55 55 60 63 64 .100 .103 LIST OF TABLES Sample Sizes . . Mean Ages and Standard Deviations Page 19 for Children 0 O O O O O O O O O I O O O C O O O 1 9 Demographic Data for the Parents Of subjects 0 O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O 20 Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 Locus of Control Scores for Children from Divorced and Intact Families . . . . . . . 31 Two-way ANOVA of CRPBI Factors and Locus of Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory Scales for Mother . . . . . . . . . . 34 Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory Scales for Father . . . . . . . . . . 35 Effects of Mother's Remarriage on CRPBI Scales for Mother . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 Effects of Custody Arrangement on CRPBI Scales for Father . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50 vii INTRODUCTION The high divorce rate in contemporary American society has raised questions about the psychological effects of divorce. Initial psychological research on divorce focused on the adult experience of marital breakup. However, the fact that millions of American children now experience the divorce of their parents has fostered investigations of the impact of parental divorce on children. The present study provides a child's-eye view of the effects of divorce. Much of the previous literature on the effects of divorce on children is based on the viewpoint of parents, teachers, or mental health professionals. Although the adult perspective continues to be useful, it is also important to understand the child's experience. In the present study, later latency children from divorced and intact families were compared in their perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control. Divorce-related variables (e.g., parental remarriage, time since divorce) were also examined for their effects on children's perceptions. The results of this study should enhance our understanding of the effects of divorce on children. STATEMENT OF PURPOSE The goal of this study was to improve our understanding of how divorce affects children. The study was conducted to provide information useful to mental health professionals, parents, teachers, and others who help children cope with the divorce of their parents. This study examined perceptions of children from divorced and intact families. A primary objective was to determine if there were differences between children from divorced and intact families in their perceptions of parental behavior. Three major components of children's perceptions of parents' behavior were studied: (1) acceptance vs rejection, (2) psychological autonomy vs psychological control, and (3) firm control vs lax control. Another primary objective was to determine if children from divorced and intact families differ in their perceived "locus of control," i.e., the extent to which they believe that their destiny is controlled by internal vs external factors. As secondary and more exploratory objectives, children's perceptions of their mothers were compared with their perceptions of their fathers, and for children of divorce, perceptions of custodial versus non-custodial parents were compared. I also tested for the effects of sex and for possible family x sex interactions on children's perceptions of parent behavior and locus of 3 control. Exploratory comparisons examined the effects of age, and for children of divorce, the effects of demographic variables (e.g., time since divorce) on children's perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control. To address the objectives described above, children of divorce were studied by comparing them to a control group of children from intact families. The sample was drawn from a normal population of children in later latency. A review of the literature relevant to the specific aspects of this study is given below. In particular, it will be demonstrated how previous studies led to the development of the present study. First, a review of research on children of divorce will be provided. Then, literature and rationale will be given for the following three specific features of the present study: 1) Later latency children of divorce; 2) Children's perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control; 3) Children of divorced and intact families from a normal population. L_ REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Background on Children of Divorce Over the last two decades, the divorce rate in the United States has increased dramatically. Current figures indicate that about 47% of new marriages will end in divorce. More than one million children a year now experience the divorce of their parents. There are approximately twenty million children of divorce in the United States today (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1980). Demographic projections estimate that in 1990, one-third of American children will have lived with a divorced parent before they are eighteen years old (Glick, 1979). Clearly, children of divorce make up a sizable part of the population and will continue to do so in the future. Children of divorce have special psychological needs and better systems for the delivery of services to meet their needs are warranted (Benedek and Benedek, 1979). However, the literature on children of divorce is relatively small. Further research is called for to better understand and serve these children. A review of past research in this area will be presented here. Levitin (1979) has divided the literature into three major research approaches: the single parent family research tradition, the clinical research tradition, and the classic studies. Single parent family research was conducted in the 1950's and 1960's and typically focused on the "father absent" household. Levitin (1979) notes that 4 5 in some cases, the cause of father absence (e.g., death, divorce) was not specified and that frequently the single parent family was regarded as a deviant form of the intact family. In contrast, there have also been studies which have not assumed that children of divorce will fare more poorly than children from intact families (For example, see reviews of Biller, 1970, 1976; Herzog and Sudia, 1973). In the clinical research tradition, subjects are drawn from a patient population. The scientist-practitioner often uses his or her own case studies of children of divorce. The focus of clinical literature has been on the symptoms displayed by the child of a divorced family. For example, Gardner (1976) provides a thorough discussion of the problems and treatments of children of divorce. Westman et a1., 1970, reported that 15% of all cases seen in a child psychiatry clinic were emotionally disturbed children of divorce. In another study of a child psychiatric population, children of divorce were distinguished from children from intact families by such symptoms as running away from home, delinquency, poor home behavior, and poor school behavior (McDermott, 1970). McDermott also found that one-third of children in the divorce group experienced moderate to severe forms of depression. In his review of the records of 400 children referred for outpatient psychiatric evaluation, Kalter (1977) reported that children of divorce appeared at almost twice the rate of their occurrence in the general 6 population. Nearly one-third of the referrals were children of divorce. The "classic studies" include two longitudinal studies which both began in the 1970's. One study by Hetherington, Cox, and Cox (1976, 1978, 1979) investigated the effects of divorce on preschool children. They matched their sample of 48 preschool children of divorce on age and sex with 48 preschool children from intact families. Hetherington et a1. noted an increase in negative behavior among children of divorce and a negative parent-child interaction in many divorce families. The mother-son relationship was especially difficult. One year after divorce, disturbances in play and social relations were observed in girls and boys of divorce. These disturbances had mainly disappeared in girls two years after the divorce. Many boys, however, continued to show developmental deviations two years following parental divorce. The second major longitudinal study of divorce is by Wallerstein and Kelly (1974, 1975, 1976, 1977, 1980, 1983). Their sample included 131 children and adolescents from 60 divorcing families in Marin County, California. Clinical interviews and preventive, child-focused intervention were provided to the children and parents at the time of parental separation. One year, five year, and ten year followups have been conducted with these families. Wallerstein and Kelly divided their sample of children by developmental stages. They report that a central 7 determinant of the child's reaction to divorce is its age and developmental stage. The Wallerstein and Kelly study is a major contribution to the literature on the impact of divorce on children. Later Latency Children of Divorce Children of later latency age (9 - 12 years old) were chosen as the target population for the present study for the following reasons: As described above, Wallerstein and Kelly found that the child's age and developmental stage greatly affect the child's response to divorce. Therefore, in studying the important problem of the effects of divorce on children, it is best to select a particular age group for study. The present study focuses on later latency children of divorce because many children referred to mental health clinics are of later latency age (9 - 12 years old), but there are few studies which examine the effects of divorce on this age group (see review by Rohrlich et a1., 1977). Further, later latency children were selected for a practical reason -- most children in this age group can read and respond to written questions. Thus, the cognitive abilities of later latency children made them excellent candidates for the present study which is based on children's reports of their perceptions of parent behavior and their locus of control. The next sections will describe the rationale for studying the effects of divorce on children's perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control. 8 Children's Perceptions of Parent Behavior and Locus of Control A major intention of this study was to obtain the child's perspective of the effects of divorce. Much of the previous research on the effects of divorce on children relies on the reports of parents, teachers or mental health professionals. Although the adult perspective is useful, it is also essential to directly examine the child's experience and viewpoint. The recent literature on children of divorce shows increasing awareness of the child's perspective (Kurdek and Berg, 1983), but many important questions remain unanswered. Therefore, the present study addresses some key questions related to two important aspects of children's perceptions: (1) children's perceptions of parent behavior and (2) children's perceptions of their own locus of control. Children's perceptions of parent behavior were studied because such perceptions are important in their own right and because children's perceptions are good indicators of many other important attributes. Schaefer (1965a) has pointed out that many studies have shown that children's reports of parental behavior are significantly related to other data on parent-child relationships (e.g., Andry, 1957; Bronson, Katten and Livson, 1959), to inventory measures of child adjustment (e.g., Berdie and Layton, 1957; Serot and Teevan, 1961), to observers' reports of child behavior (Brown et a1., 1947; Bronfenbrenner, 1961), to school achievement (Morrow and Wilson, 1961) and to 9 other criteria of the child's adjustment (Ausubel et a1., 1954; Cooper and Blair, 1959). Studies have also shown that children's reports of parent behavior differentiate normal subjects from psychiatric patients (e.g., Greenfield, 1959; Garmezy et a1., 1961). In some cases, the child's adjustment may be more closely related to the child's perception of its parents' behavior than to the actual parental behavior (Schaefer, 1965a). Further, I chose to study children's perceptions of parent behavior because there is evidence suggesting that this parameter may be affected by divorce, as described below. Previous studies have described various negative effects of divorce on later latency children that have been viewed as characteristic of the later latency child's level of self-reflective social reasoning (Longfellow, 1979). From their study of 31 later latency children (age 9 - 10), Wallerstein and Kelly (1976) concluded that later latency children of divorce were most clearly distinguished by their intense anger towards one or both parents. Other reactions of the later latency child included fears of being unloved and abandoned (Wallerstein and Kelly, 1976). Wallerstein and Kelly (1976) also reported that parents were often so involved with their own problems during divorce that there was a sharp withdrawal of interest in their children. In sum, previous work suggests that later latency children of divorce experience anger towards parents, fear 10 of rejection, and withdrawal of parental interest. These experiences would be expected to influence the later latency child's perception of parental acceptance/ rejection. Thus, a key issue addressed in the present study is the effect of parental divorce on the later latency child's perception of parental acceptance/ rejection. Results from previous work, as described above, suggest that divorce may cause later latency children to perceive their parents as less accepting/more rejecting. This is the first major hypothesis tested in the present study. Weiss (1979) proposed that the echelon structure of a two-parent household enables children to remain dependent. In contrast, he noted that children growing up in single- parent households displayed an earlier maturity prompted by demands on them for autonomy and responsibility. If these demands for autonomy are reflected in the later latency child's perceptions of parent behavior, then children of divorce would be expected to perceive their parents as less controlling and more granting of autonomy to the child. This idea was tested as the second major hypothesis in the present study. It has been suggested that divorced parents often experience difficulty in exercising authority and firm discipline (Hetherington, 1979; Tooley, 1975). Thus, the later latency child's perceptions of parents' disciplinary control may be affected by divorce. The third major 11 hypothesis tested in the present study is that later latency children of divorce perceive their parents as exercising less disciplinary control. Some investigators have suggested that children of divorce are likely to develop external frames of reference, because a variety of events during divorce are outside the child's control (e.g., parental separation and divorce, changes in residence, school, and income; Duke and Lancaster, 1976; Kulka and Weingarten, 1979; Wallerstein and Kelly, 1980). If this suggestion is true, children of divorce would be expected to have an external locus of control. Thus, the fourth major hypothesis tested here was that later latency children of divorce develop external locus of control. Previous studies have demonstrated that parental divorce may affect boys differently than girls (Hetherington, 1979; Wallerstein and Kelly, 1980; Kurdek and Berg, 1983; Slater et a1., 1983). Therefore, in testing the four primary hypotheses described above, the sex of the child was taken into consideration. Children ofyDiyorced and Intact Families from a Normal Population There have been many excellent studies of children and divorce, but most have looked only at children from divorced families without inclusion of a control group (e.g., Wallerstein and Kelly, 1980; Kurdek et a1., 1981; Hingst, 1982). The present study examined the effect of 12 divorce pg; gg by comparing children from divorced families with a control group of children from intact families. An "intact" family is defined here as a family in which: (1) both of the child's biological parents are present, and (2) there has never been a legal divorce. It is recognized that the parents in an "intact" family may be "emotionally divorced" (Despert, 1962), but for the purposes of this study the variable of legal divorce was examined. Thus, "children of divorce" are defined as children whose parents are legally divorced and no longer live together. Levitin (1979) observed that "since the children who are in therapy are apt to be the most distressed of children, the clinical literature on children of divorce describes the most extreme and pathological patterns of response." By using a normal population in the present study, the potentially confounding effect of interviewing the most disturbed children was reduced. Sampling_and the Problem of Generalizability As stated above, the present study improves upon prior research by studying a non-clinic sample of children of divorce in comparison with a control group of children from intact families. However, there is another problem with the studies reviewed that also applies to the present work. Families of divorce approached to participate in research frequently refuse to do so. There are many reasons why 13 this may be so: Concern that such research may complicate the process of adjustment for children, defensiveness in the face of still-prevalent stigmatization of families of divorce, reluctance to cope with yet another demand on strained family resources. For those families that participate, there is no way of knowing how representative they are of families of divorce in general. Are they among those which are not coping well but hope to obtain some help through the research process? Are they among those which are coping well and are proud enough of the fact to want to show others how well they are doing? Clearly, either of these descriptions would imply quite different family characteristics and therefore quite different interpretations of research findings. This problem has not been sufficiently acknowledged in prior research reports. Given the nature of divorce and the difficulties faced by divorcing families, it may be a functionally unresolvable problem. In any case, the results not only of the present study but of prior research must be read with this limitation on generalizability in mind. HYPOTHESES The general trend in the literature suggests that divorce will have a negative impact on children. Therefore, the theme underlying the major hypotheses in this study was that later latency children of divorce would differ from children from intact families by showing more negative perceptions of parent behavior and more external locus of control. Based on the literature reviewed in the previous section, the following four major hypotheses were tested: 1. Later latency children of divorce will perceive their parents as less accepting/more rejecting. 2. Later latency children of divorce will perceive their parents as less controlling and more granting of psychological autonomy to the child. 3. Later latency children of divorce will perceive their parents as exercising less disciplinary control. 4. Later latency children of divorce will show an external locus of control. While testing the four major hypotheses, the effects of sex of the child were also examined. The first three hypotheses are related to the three primary factors of the Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory (CRPBI): (1) Acceptance vs Rejection, (2) Psychological Control vs Psychological Autonomy, and (3) Firm Control vs Lax Control (Schaefer, 1965a, 1965b). Therefore, the first three hypotheses predict that later 14 15 latency children of divorce will perceive their parents as lower on the Acceptance, Psychological Control, and Firm Control factors. Definitions for the three CRPBI factors and descriptions of the CRPBI scales which load on each factor are given below in the Method section. The fourth hypothesis relates to children's locus of control. As corollaries to the three primary hypotheses relating to the three factors of the CRPBI, subsidiary hypotheses relating to the 18 scales of the CRPBI were also tested. It was hypothesized that scales loading on a particular factor would reflect the differences expected for that factor. For example, the Acceptance scale has a positive loading on the Acceptance-Rejection factor. Thus, in line with the first major hypothesis, children of divorce were expected to be lower on the Acceptance scale. In addition to the four major hypotheses and the corollary hypotheses, a number of exploratory comparisons were made. Children's perceptions of their mothers were compared with their perceptions of their fathers. Other exploratory analyses looked at the effects of age, and for the divorced group, the effects of demographic variables on children's perceptions of parent behavior and locus of control. For children of divorce, there were also comparisons of the children's perceptions of custodial vs non-custodial parents. METHOD Design A two-way design was used. The first independent variable was the child's family type: divorced versus intact. The second independent variable was sex of the child. Among the children from divorced families, there were eight other independent variables: mother's remarriage, father's remarriage, custody arrangement, visitation by the non-custodial parent, siblings living with the child, years since divorce, years since mother's remarriage, and years since father's remarriage. The dependent variables were the children's perceptions of parent behavior as indicated by their responses to the Children's Reports of Parental Behavior Inventory (CRPBI) devised by Schaefer (1965a) and the children's locus of control as measured by the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale (1973). Subjects Sixty-eight 9-12 year old subjects participated in this study. Criteria for inclusion of the subject were: 1) age between 9-12 years, 2) family structure defined as intact or divorced, 3) ability to read and write in English, and 4) residence with at least one of the child's biological parents. The definitions of intact vs divorced family structure (See Review of the Literature) were 16 17 strictly adhered to. Adopted children were excluded from the study. Subjects were recruited from the East Lansing/Lansing, Michigan area and other surrounding communities. Recruitment of subjects was arranged with the cooperation of church and synagogue youth education groups, the Haslett Public Schools, and the Ingham County Friend of the Court. Class lists and addresses of families with age appropriate subjects were obtained from the above groups. A letter to parents (See Appendix C) explaining the study was sent to approximately one thousand families. Parents were asked to return an enclosed postcard indicating whether they were interested in participating with their children. Of the approximately 1000 initial contacts, 300 postcards were returned, yielding a 30% return rate. About one half of these postcards were refusals and the other half were positive replies. Interested parents were contacted by phone to answer further questions about the study. If selection criteria were satisfied after the telephone screening, appointment times were set up. From the pool of 300 returned postcards, a total of 66 potential subjects were eliminated due to selection criteria (e.g., age, family type, adoption) with the majority excluded because the children didn't meet the age criteria. Another 42 potential subjects from intact families were screened by telephone and appeared to satisfy the selection criteria. However, these children were not 18 tested because there was already an adequate number of subjects from intact families. All of the appropriate subjects from divorced families were tested. The relatively low number of appropriate subjects obtained from divorced families may have been due to a low frequency of divorced families in the population contacted and/or a lower rate of responding. Following screening, seventy-two subjects were tested. However, two subjects were discarded after testing because they were deemed inappropriate by the selection criteria. Another two subjects were lost when the procedure criteria were not satisfied. The final sample was composed of 68 subjects from middle class neighborhoods. Sample sizes for subject groups by family type and sex are given in Table 1. Subjects ranged in age from 9.0 to 12.9 years. See Table 2 for mean ages and standard deviations by family type and sex. Eighty-six percent of the children from divorced families had siblings; 92% of the children from intact families had siblings. Within the divorced group, the time since parental divorce ranged from one to eleven years. For girls from divorced families, the mean number of years since parental divorce was 6.4 years (SD = 2.8 years). There was a mean time of 6.1 years (SD = 2.7 years) since parental divorce for boys. Children's custody arrangements included: 82% mother custody, 11% father custody, and 7% joint custody. 19 Table 1. Sample Sizes Divorced Intact Total Boys 15 19 34 Girls 13 21 34 Total 28 40 68 Table 2. Mean Ages and Standard Deviations for Children Divorced Intact Total Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Boys 11.1 1.0 11.2 1.0 11.1 1.0 Girls 11.5 1.0 11.1 1.2 11.2 1.2 Total 11.2 1.0 11.1 1.1 11.2 1.1 20 In: nun wmv «mm coauumemu aaucmuuoo mmm mom emu Sam mumsomumumom ANN wov yam we. mmeafloo wmm won see we» Hoosom amen scaumoscm em . mm mm . om me u on «a n on mmamu mme m.mm m.mm ¢.Am «.mm one new: umsumm nonuoz umnumm nacho: uomucH ooouo>wo muomnnsm mo mucoumm may now mama canmmumOEon .m OHQMB 21 Children's current living arrangements showed 79% living with mother, 14% living with father, and 7% living alternately with mother and father. About 89% of the children were visited by their non-custodial parent. Demographic data for the parents of the 68 subjects appear in Table 3. For the intact group, the mean length of parents' marriage at the time of the study was 15.9 years (range: 12 - 24 years). Procedure All subjects were tested in their homes at the convenience of the parents and children. It was required that a parent be at home during testing. Informed consent was obtained with the parent's signature on an explanatory consent form prior to their child's participation. Verbal assent was obtained from the children. Children and parents were assured that their responses were strictly confidential. Parents were then asked to fill out a demographic survey form containing such information as family type (divorced versus intact), custody arrangement, time since divorce, and number and ages of children. A copy of the parent form is found in Appendix D. After the parent's forms were completed, the experimenter tested the child in a quiet, private room. There was a brief initial period during which the experimenter conversed and established rapport with the child. A short explanation was given by the experimenter to every child about the researcher's interest in studying 22 children's feelings about their parents. The CRPBI and the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale were administered to each child with standardized instructions. The entire administration per subject lasted approximately one hour. Instruments Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory (CRPBI) The accumulating evidence of the validity of children's reports of parental behavior motivated Schaefer (1965a) to develop "short, reliable scales for a systematic sample of parental-behavior concepts"; thus he devised an instrument called the Children's Reports of Parental Behavior Inventory (CRPBI). The present study employed Schaefer's CRPBI, because unlike earlier inventories, it describes the child-parent interaction rather than marital/parental adjustment. This important feature enabled a focus on the child's perspective. The CRPBI also distinguishes maternal from paternal behavior, which is necessary to compare children's perceptions of mothers vs fathers and custodial vs non-custodial parents, as stated in the secondary objectives (see above). Schaefer (1965b) reported a configurational analysis of the CRPBI in which he found three replicated factors: Acceptance versus Rejection, Psychological Autonomy versus Psychological Control, and Firm Control versus Lax Control. These factors were used in the present study to compare children from divorced and intact families. 23 The original CRPBI is composed of 26 scales which each contain 10 items. Schludermann and Schludermann (1970) developed a shortened form of the CRPBI which consists of 18 scales. Six of these scales contain 8 items per scale; the other 12 scales contain 5 items per scale. They chose the 18 scales using the criteria of high scale reliability, variability, and applicability to parental behavior. Those items which were inappropriate for ethnic and religious minority groups were eliminated in order to make the instrument suitable for diverse groups. The same 108 items were used on separate maternal and paternal forms. Schludermann and Schludermann (1970) conducted a methodological study of the factor structure of the modified CRPBI using two different independent samples. They found that the three factors (Acceptance versus Rejection, Psychological Autonomy versus Psychological Control, and Firm Control versus Lax Control) were highly replicable across parental forms, sex groups, and independent samples. Since the CRPBI factors showed such a high replicability, Schludermann and Schludermann recommended using the three factors to describe results rather than reporting the 18 scale scores (Schludermann and Schludermann, 1970, 1971, 1983). "The Acceptance vs Rejection factor describes the subject's perception of different degrees of parental acceptance or rejection" (Schludermann and Schludermann, 1983). The scales with positive loadings on the Acceptance 24 vs Rejection factor are Acceptance, Childcenteredness, Possessiveness, Positive Involvement, and Acceptance of Individuation; the Rejection and Hostile Detachment scales load negatively on this factor. The Psychological Control vs Psychological Autonomy factor is defined by scales which "describe covert, psychological methods of controlling the child's activities and behaviors that would not permit the child to develop as an individual apart from the parent" (Schaefer, 1965b). The following scales which comprise the Psychological Control vs Psychological Autonomy factor all have positive loadings on this factor: Intrusiveness, Control through Guilt, Hostile Control, Inconsistent Discipline, Instilling Persistent Anxiety, and Withdrawal of Relations. The Firm Control vs Lax Control factor "indicates the degree to which the parent makes rules and regulations, sets limits to the child's activities, and enforces these rules and limits" (Schaefer, 1965b). Scales loading positively on this factor are Control and Enforcement. The Nonenforcement, Lax Discipline, and Extreme Autonomy scales have negative loadings on the Firm Control vs Lax Control factor. The CRPBI has been administered in such diverse studies as the following: normal school children and delinquent boys (Schaefer, 1965a); French-speaking Belgian high school students (Renson et a1., 1968); Manitoban university students (Schludermann and Schludermann, 1970); 25 Canadian Hutterite adolescents (Schludermann and Schludermann, 1971); and Canadian and Indian adolescents (Schludermann and Schludermann, 1983). The three basic factor dimensions were replicated in all studies. There appears to be good evidence for the CRPBI's cross-cultural validity (Renson et a1., 1968; Schludermann and Schludermann, 1971, 1983). The shortened 108-item version of Schaefer's Children's Reports of Parent Behavior Inventory (CRPBI) was used to measure children's perceptions of their parents' behavior (Schaefer, 1965a, 1965b; Schludermann and Schludermann, 1970, 1971, 1983). The 108 items describing parental behavior were found on separate but equivalent forms for mother and father. The language on the maternal and paternal forms was appropriate for the parent's gender. Subjects were instructed to rate each item as "Like," "Somewhat Like," or "Not Like" their parent's behavior (e.g., "Gives me a lot of care and attention"; "Is very strict with me"). Every child was reminded to report on his or her biological mother and father. The maternal and paternal forms of the CRPBI were administered to subjects in counterbalanced order. Items on the CRPBI received scoring values ("Like" = 3; "Somewhat Like" a 2; and "Not Like" a 1). These item scores were totaled and multiplied by a coefficient to determine the scale scores. Factor scores were obtained by adding designated scale scores and dividing by the number of scales. Each child received 3 26 factor scores and 18 scale scores for both the maternal and paternal forms of the CRPBI. See Appendix A for a copy of the CRPBI, a sample scoring sheet, and complete scoring instructions. Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale The "internal-external locus of control" construct was derived from social learning theory (Rotter, 1954, 1966). MacDonald (1973) summarized the construct in the following way: "Internal-external locus of control refers to the extent to which persons perceive contingency relationships between their actions and their outcomes." At one end of the scale are "Internals" who believe that they have some control over their destinies. In contract, "Externals" believe that their outcomes are determined by factors extrinsic to themselves, such as luck or powerful others. There is a large literature on the locus of control construct which MacDonald (1973) attributes to its generalizability and social relevance. Internal locus of control is seen as a distinct social advantage. For example, significant relationships between internal locus of control and achievement have been found (Coleman et a1., 1966; Nowicki and Roundtree, 1971). The locus of control variable has been shown to have considerable relationship to children's behavior. One of the reasons this study included a locus of control measure is because previous investigators have found that locus of control is a significant predictor of children's divorce 27 adjustment. Children with internal locus of control orientations were more likely to show better adjustment to their parents' divorce (Kurdek et a1., 1981). Other studies have suggested that internal locus of control (especially for boys) is significantly related to academic achievement, social maturity, and independent, self- motivated behavior (Nowicki and Strickland, 1973). Researchers have developed instruments to study the locus of control dimension in children (see Bialer, 1961; Battle and Rotter, 1963; Crandell et a1., 1965; Nowicki and Strickland, 1973). In his 1973 review, MacDonald remarked that: "The Nowicki and Strickland Locus of Control Scale is a 40-item yes-no paper-pencil test that has been used extensively with subjects ranging from the third grade through college. Information on the scale's internal consistency reliability, test-retest reliability, and convergent and discriminant validity indicates it to be the best measure of locus of control as a generalized expectancy presently available for use with children." The Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale was used because of its strength as a reliable and valid measure. Another positive feature was the short form of the instrument recommended by the authors, which was employed in this study to reduce the demand on subjects. The Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale was administered last to each child. This 40-item scale required the child to answer "yes" or "no" to straight- forward questions (e.g., "Do you believe that most problems will solve themselves if you just don't fool with them?"). 28 Items were scored using a point system to derive a locus of control score for each subject. See Appendix B for a copy of the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale. RESULTS The results relating to the four primary hypotheses are described below. Next, results relating to the corollary hypotheses are described. Finally, results of the exploratory comparisons are presented. Exploratory comparisons are reported in the following sections: comparisons between mother and father, effects of age, and effects within the divorced group. Comparisons between Children from Divorced and Intact Families Primary analyses: CRPBI factors and locus of control Means and standard deviations for the CRPBI factors and locus of control appear in Tables 4 and 5, respectively. The results show that in the present study children of divorce and children from intact families did not differ significantly in perceptions of parent behavior as measured by the three CRPBI factors. Two-way ANOVA showed no significant main effects of family type for the three CRPBI factors for mother or for father (See Table 6). These results do not support the three primary hypotheses predicting differences between children from divorced families and children from intact families. The results also show no significant difference between children from divorced and intact families in locus of control. Two-way ANOVA showed no significant main effect of family type on locus of control (See Table 6). 29 30 m.~ o.m~ o.m F.P~ m.~ P.0N m.~ m.P~ nonumm P.m m.~m m.~ m.Pm m.m m.P~ m.~ F.NN wonuoz Howucou xmq u Honucou Ewan F.N ~.¢F ~.~ P.vp o.~ P.¢P >.— m.vp wonumm m.m o.mp m.~ —.mP m.~ o.mp ~.N h.mp nosuoz meocousd HobamoHono>mm I Houucou HobamoHonoamm F.v m.v~ N.m F.v~ o.m m.¢~ m.F h.mN Hmcumm N.¢ >.v~ v.m m.mN F.~ m.¢~ m.P h.m~ Hocuoz c0wuomfiom|oocmummoo< am com: um coo: am now: am coo: Houomm mauaw whom maufiw mmom uomucH poouo>ao mucuomm muoucm>cH uofl>mnmm ucmumm mo manommm m.cmuoafinu .v manna 31 Table 5. Locus of Control Scores for Children from Divorced and Intact Families Divorced Intact Boys Girls Boys Girls Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD 12.7 4.2 13.1 3.6 11.8 5.0 11.7 4.1 This observed lack of difference between children from divorced and intact families conflicts with the hypothesis predicting that children of divorce would have more external locus of control. In summary, the four primary hypotheses predicting differences between children from divorced and intact families were not supported by the data. Two-way ANOVA also showed no significant effects of sex and no significant family type x sex interactions on either the CRPBI factors or the locus of control (See Table 6). 32 Table 6. Two-way ANOVA of CRPBI Factors and Locus of Control Probability Values for Effects* Factor Family Sex Family x Sex Acceptance-Rejection Mother 0.68 0.36 0.96 Father 0.42 0.79 0.18 Psychological Control - Psychological Autonomy Mother 0.37 0.84 0.98 Father 0.54 0.63 0.41 Firm Control - Lax Control Mother 0.52 0.91 0.41 Father 0.22 0.87 0.16 Locus of Control 0.29 0.89 0.80 * No differences were significant at p < 0.05. 33 Secondary analyses: Individual CRPBI scale score; Means and standard deviations for the individual CRPBI scales are given in Tables 7 and 8. Results for each CRPBI scale for each parent were analyzed by two-way ANOVA to test for main effects of the two independent variables family type and sex and for potential family type x sex interactions. The data for boys and girls were also analyzed separately by one-way ANOVA to test for effects of family type on each sex independently. By making these numerous comparisons, the probability that true differences go undetected is reduced, but the chance for type I errors is increased. Therefore, interpretation of the results will focus on differences that are highly significant or that represent a pattern in the results for both parents. The two-way ANOVA showed no effects of family type or sex for 17 of the 18 CRPBI scales for mother and father. The one exceptional scale showing significant main effects was Possessiveness. The means for Possessiveness were significantly higher for children of divorce than for children from intact families for both mothers, F(1,64) = 9.5, p < 0.005, and for fathers, F(1,64) = 4.6, p < 0.05. One-way ANOVA yielded significant main effects of family type for boys on 2 of the 18 CRPBI scales for mother. The means were significantly higher for boys of divorce than for boys from intact families on mothers' Possessiveness, F(1,32) = 4.3, p < 0.05, and on mothers' Instilling Persistent Anxiety, F(1,32) = 5.1, p < 0.05. 34 ~.m m.mP m.m ~.4. m.m m.4p A.4 n.4F meocouaa msmuuxm m.m P.m. m.~ P.mp 4.m n.4p A.m m.~. mcofiumflwm «0 Haamuonufiz 4.4 m.~P k.~ w.PP m.~ m.~F ~.P A... ucmssomuwa maflumom m.m «.4, A.m m.~. ~.m ~.4. m.m m.mp sumfixc< bemumflmumm acfiflflfiumcH ~.4 4.4F m.m P.5F 4.4 m.np 4.m m.a_ ocaaaflomfio xaq 5.4 ..mm o.4 m.m~ m.m m.m~ m.~ m.m~ :04uman>HcEH mo moemuaoooa m.m m.mP m.4 m.mp ..4 4.m. 4.m n.4, unmemououcmcoz «.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. o.4 ~.m. 4.m «.m. ocaaauomfio bemumfimaoocH o.m n.4, A.m m.m. o.m n.4, a.~ «.4, Houucoo wfiflumom o.m m.mp m.4 P.m. _.m 4.mp m.4 P.mP uHHsu nmsounu Houueoo m.m n.5, 4.4 4.4, o.4 ~.m. o.4 n.4, mmmcm>nmsuucH 4.4 ..o~ ~.m 4.m~ ..m m.o~ 4.~ m.o~ unwem>ao>cH m>auumom m.4 o.AP m.m m.4P m.m c.4P 4.4 «.A4 bememououcm m.4 m.o~ 5.4 A.aP m.m m.m. m.4 m.F~ Houucou m.4 m.mp A.~ m.~F o.m P.4P 4.~ m.m. couuomflmm ..m m.AF m.4 4.4. o.m o.o~ m.~ m.F~ mmwam>ammommom m.m m.m~ o.4 c.4m 4.m n.4m ..4 m.4~ mmmcomumuemooafino ~.m ~.4~ o.m m.m~ m.~ m.o~ 4.m m.4~ mocmuamou< am GMT: Om Gmwz Om 6mm: Om GMT: wdmom maufiw m>om mauau whom HOGUCH ©00HO>HQ Honuoz you moamom auouco>cH uoH>mnmm acmumm mo muuommm m.counafico .n magma uwzuoa you mwacom >u0ucm>CH u04>m£0m ucmumm MO muhOQOm m.:muh~w£U .m ONQME 35 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.5. 4.4 4.4. 450:0444 0204044 5.4 5.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4... 4.4 4.4. 4:0.04404 40 443040:043 5.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 ..4. 4.. 4... 0402400000 0440404 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4004444 4404444404 4444440444 4.4 4.5. 4.4 4.5. 4.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 0444440444 444 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 40.04004>4004 40 0044040004 4.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.5. 4.4 4.4. 0:050000440402 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 0444440444 000044040044 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4000400 0440404 4.4 4.4. 4.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 44.00 2440040 4004400 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 ..5. 4.4 4.4. 44040>4400444 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 ..4 4.44 4.. 4.54 Damsm>40>04 0>404404 4.4 4.5. ..4 4.4. ..4 4.4. 4.4 5.4. 00050000444 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.4. 4.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 4040:00 4.4 4.4. 5.4 4.4. 4.4 5.4. 4.. 5... 404000404 4.4 4.5. 4.4 5.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 44040>44404404 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 4.4 ..44 440000000400044s0 ..4 4.44 4.4 4.44 4.4 4.44 4.4 5.44 00:0000004 am com: am com: am new: om coo: mamom maufiw mmom wauau whom DomucH omouo>4o Monumm How mmamom huouco>cH uofi>msom ucmwmm mo muwomom m.:0H©H4nU .m manna 36 There was only one CRPBI scale for mother for which one-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect of family type for girls. Girls from divorced families had significantly higher means for mothers' Possessiveness than did girls from intact families, F(1,32) = 5.4, p < 0.05. A significant main effect of family type for boys was revealed by one-way ANOVA on just one CRPBI scale for father. On the fathers' Positive Involvement scale, boys from divorced families had significantly higher means than boys from intact families, F(1,32) = 6.1, p < 0.05. One-way ANOVA also showed a significant main effect of family for girls on only one CRPBI scale for father. The means for fathers' Enforcement were significantly lower for girls of divorce than for girls from intact families, F(1,32) = 5.1, p < 0.05. In summary, there were few significant differences between children from divorced and intact families in their perceptions of parent behavior as indicated by the CRPBI scales. Possessiveness was the only CRPBI scale which showed significant main effects of family type for boys and girls combined. Overall, children of divorce perceived both their mothers and fathers as significantly more possessive than did children from intact families. The probability that both of these differences occurred by chance is less than 0.00025. Thus, the difference in the Possessiveness scale most likely reflects a true difference between the children from divorced and intact families. 37 Since the Possessiveness scale has a positive loading on the Acceptance vs Rejection factor, this result was in the opposite direction of the corollary hypothesis that children of divorce would perceive their parents as less accepting and hence less possessive. When the data were analyzed separately for boys and girls, 4 of the 5 significant main effects of family type on the CRPBI scales were also in the opposite direction of the corollary hypotheses. However, the finding that girls of divorce perceived their fathers as significantly lower on the Enforcement scale than girls from intact families supported a corollary hypothesis; the Enforcement scale has a positive loading on the Firm Control factor and children of divorce were hypothesized to be lower on the Firm Control factor. Thus, the corollary hypotheses were generally not supported by the data because there were few effects of family type on the CRPBI scales and because nearly all of the effects that were observed were not in the expected direction. Exploratory Comparisong Comparisons between Mother and Father Paired t-tests were used to compare mothers' and fathers' CRPBI factor scores. Girls of divorce perceived their mothers as significantly higher on the Psychological Control factor than their fathers (t = 2.6, df = 12, p < 0.03). On the Firm Control factor, girls of divorce also 38 saw their mothers as significantly higher than their fathers (t = 2.2, df = 12, p = 0.05). Boys of divorce viewed their mothers as significantly higher than fathers on the Psychological Control factor (t = 2.4, df = 14, p < 0.05). Girls from intact families perceived no significant differences between mothers and fathers. They perceived mother and father as similar on all three factors. However, boys from intact families gave their mothers significantly higher Psychological Control factor scores than their fathers (t = 3.0, df = 18, p < 0.01). It is striking that all children except girls from intact families saw mother as significantly higher than father on the Psychological Control factor. Mother versus father scale scores on the CRPBI were compared using paired t-tests. Girls from divorced families viewed their mothers as exerting significantly greater Hostile Control than their fathers (t = 2.8, df = 12, p < 0.02). The girls from divorced families also perceived their mothers as significantly higher than their fathers on two other scales: Instilling Persistent Anxiety (t = 2.4, df = 12, p < 0.05) and Withdrawal of Relations (t = 3.2, df = 12, p < 0.01). Boys of divorced families gave their mothers significantly higher scores on the Rejection scale than their fathers (t = 3.6, df = 14, p < 0.005). On the Instilling Persistent Anxiety scale, boys from divorced 39 families saw their mothers as significantly higher than their fathers (t = 3.1, df = 14, p < 0.01). There were no significant differences on mother versus father CRPBI scale scores for girls from intact families. These girls saw their mothers and fathers as alike on all 18 scales! Boys from intact families reported the most differences in their perceptions of mother vs father. The boys from intact families viewed their mothers as significantly higher than their fathers on the following five scales: Possessiveness (t a 2.5, df = 18, p < 0.05), Positive Involvement (t = 3.0, df = 18, p < 0.01), Intrusiveness (t = 3.0, df = 18, p < 0.01), Hostile Control (t = 2.6, df = 18, p < 0.02), and Instilling Persistent Anxiety (t a 2.1, df = 18, p < 0.05). Note that on the Instilling Persistent Anxiety scale mother's scores were significantly higher than father's for all children except girls from intact families. Effects of Age Regression analysis of the CRPBI scales was performed to determine effects of age. For children of divorced families, perceptions of maternal Rejection increased as age increased, F(1,26) = 9.0, p < 0.01. Also, for girls from divorced families, mothers' Rejection scores increased with the girls' ages, F(1,11) = 8.4, p < 0.02. There were no other age effects on any maternal scales for children from intact or divorced families. 40 On the CRPBI scales for father, regression analysis revealed more effects of age. Paternal Acceptance scores decreased as the age of children from divorced families increased, F(1,26) = 6.2, p < 0.02. Children from intact families did not show an age effect on paternal Acceptance. Another age effect was seen in the fathers' Acceptance of Individuation scale for boys but not for girls. As age increased for boys, the fathers' Acceptance of Individuation scores increased, F(1,32) = 4.2, p < 0.05. Three strong age effects were found for boys of divorce on the paternal scales. Fathers' Acceptance decreased as age increased for boys of divorce, F(1,13) = 6.0, p < 0.03. As the age of divorced boys increased, paternal Possessiveness also increased, F(1,13) = 6.8, p = 0.02. Scores on the fathers' Acceptance of Individuation scale increased as age increased for boys of divorce, F(1,13) = 6.4, p < 0.03. It is striking that there were strong effects of age on the paternal CRPBI scales for boys of divorced families but there were no significant age effects for girls of divorced families. For girls of intact families, fathers' Extreme Autonomy decreased as the girls' ages increased, F(1,19) = 5.8, p < 0.03. In addition to this strong effect, a weaker age effect was found on the paternal Hostile Control scale. As the age of girls from intact families increased, fathers' Hostile Control scores decreased, F(1,19) = 4.3, p 41 = 0.05. There were no age effects on the paternal scales for boys from intact families. Overall, there was a pattern in which children of divorce showed age effects in their perceptions of parental rejection while children from intact families did not show such age effects. With only one exception (fathers' Acceptance of Individuation for boys), all significant age effects for children of divorce showed increasing parental rejection with increasing age. From the pattern of results observed here, it may be worth investigating further the effects of age on the perceptions of children of divorce. No age effects were evident on the locus of control measure for children from divorced families, for children from intact families, or for boys or girls. Effects within the Divorced Group The variables of mother's remarriage, father's remarriage, custody arrangement, visitation by the non- custodial parent, and siblings living with the child were crossed with sex of child in separate two-way ANOVAs to test for effects on the CRPBI factors and on the CRPBI scales. As noted above, there were no main effects of sex of child on the CRPBI factors or scales. The results reported here include effects of the aforementioned divorce variables and divorce variable x sex interaction effects. In addition, correlation analysis was used to examine the relationship between the CRPBI scales and years since 42 divorce, years since mother's remarriage, and years since father's remarriage. Effects of Demographic Variables on Children's Perceptions of Mother There were no significant main effects of mother's remarriage on any of the three factors for mother. However, a mother's remarriage x sex of child interaction was seen on the maternal Acceptance factor, F(1,24) = 4.3, p < 0.05. If mother was remarried, boys saw mothers as lower on Acceptance than boys whose mothers were not remarried; the reverse pattern was true for girls. The variable of father's remarriage showed no significant main or interaction effects on the three factors for mother. Custody arrangements yielded a significant main effect on the maternal Firm Control factor. On the Firm Control dimension, scores for mother were highest for children in mother custody (M a 22.7), intermediate for children in joint custody (M = 18.6), and lowest for children in father custody (M = 17.3), F(2,22) = 6.1, p < 0.01. There were no significant main effects of visitation by the non-custodial parent on the three maternal factors. A visitation by non-custodial parent x sex interaction was revealed on the maternal Acceptance factor, F(1,24) = 4.2, p = 0.05. Boys who were visited by the non-custodial parent saw mothers as higher on the Acceptance factor than boys who were not visited. In contrast, the girls who were 43 visited by their non-custodial parent gave lower maternal Acceptance scores than girls who were not visited. No main effects of living with siblings were present on the three maternal factors. There was a living with siblings x sex interaction on the maternal Firm Control factor, F(1,24) = 5.9, p < 0.03. For boys who lived with siblings, maternal Firm Control was higher than for boys who did not live with siblings. Girls living with siblings gave lower maternal Firm Control scores than girls not living with siblings. There were no significant main effects of mother's remarriage for any of the 18 scales for mother, but mother's remarriage x sex interactions, F(1,24), p < 0.05, were found on four of the scales for mother (See Table 9). For boys, Acceptance, Childcenteredness, Acceptance of Individuation, and Lax Discipline scores for mother were lower for remarried mothers than for mothers that were not remarried. Girls showed the opposite pattern on these four scales; girls awarded higher scores to their mothers if they were remarried. 44 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. ..4 4.4. 4.4 0.4. 00000000 0000000 4.4 4.4. 0.4 4.4. 4.4 0.4. ..4 0.4. 000000000 00 0030000003 5.4 0.4. 4.. 4... 4.4 4.4. 0.. 4.4. 0000000000 000040: 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 0.4. 4.4 4.4. 4000000 0000000000 0000000400 5.4 0.4. 0.4 4.5. 0.4 0..4 ..4 4.4. 0000000400 000 4.4 4.44 4.. 4.54 4.4 4.44 4.. 4.44 00000000>0000 00 0000000000 0.4 0.4. 4.4 4.4. 0.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 00000000000002 4.4 4.4. 5.4 4.4. 0.4 0.5. 0.4 4.4. 0000000400 000000000000 0.4 5.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.5. 4.4 0.4. 0000000 0000400 0.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 5.5 0.4. 5.4 0.4. 00000 0000000 0000000 4.4 5.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 0.5. 4.4 4..4 44000>0000000 ..4 4.44 0.4 ..54 4.4 ..44 4.4 0.44 00000>00>00 0>000000 4.4 4.4. 5.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 5.4 0.4. 00000000000 5.4 4.4. 4.4 4.0. 0.4 0.0. 0.4 4.44 0000000 4.4 4.4. 4.4 4.4. 4.4 ..4. 4.4 4.4. 000000400 4.4 4.04 4.4 4..4 0.. 4..4 0.4 4..4 04000>00000000 4.4 ..44 4.4 0.44 4.. 0.54 4.4 4.44 00000000000000000 4.4 0.44 4.. 4.54 4.0 4.54 4.4 4.44 0000000000 0m :mmz Gm GMT: Om GMT—z Gm GMT: Tamom 00000 m>om 00000 maom 000uumsom 002 002002 peauumeom 00:00: 00:00: 00m moamom Hmmmu :o 000000050m 0.00:002 mo muomuum .m manme 45 Father's remarriage affected the Enforcement and Instilling Persistent Anxiety scales for mother. The mean Enforcement score for mother was higher if the father was remarried (M = 18.7) than if he was not (M = 15.1), F(1,24) = 6.6, p < 0.02. The mean Instilling Persistent Anxiety score for mother was also higher if father was remarried (M = 17.2) than if he was not remarried (M = 13.5), F(1,24) = 7.2, p < 0.02. The other 16 scales for mother were unaffected by father's remarriage. There was significant variation in scores for mother's Inconsistent Discipline, Lax Discipline, and Extreme Autonomy among children living in different custody arrangements. Inconsistent Discipline scores for mother were higher for children in joint custody (M = 22.0) than for children in mother (M a 14.6) or father custody (M = 15.3), F(2,22) = 4.8, p < 0.02. For maternal Lax Discipline, children in mother custody (M = 16.5) reported lower scores than children in joint (M = 22.0) or father custody (M = 18.7), F(2,22) = 3.6, p < 0.05. Extreme Autonomy scores for mother were highest for children in joint custody (M = 23.0), intermediate for children in father custody (M = 20.7), and lowest for children in mother custody (M = 14.5), F(2,22) = 6.8, p = 0.005. No significant main effects of visitation by the non- custodial parent occurred on any of the 18 scales for mother. However, an interaction effect of visitation by the non-custodial parent x sex was found on the 46 Childcenteredness scale for mother, F(1,24) = 6.2, p = 0.02. Boys who were visited by their non-custodial parent perceived mother as higher on Childcenteredness than boys who were not visited. In contrast, girls who were visited by the non-custodial parent gave mother a lower Childcenteredness score than girls who were not visited. Children who lived with siblings reported a significantly higher score for mother on Lax Discipline (M = 18.3) than children who did not live with siblings (M = 14.2), F(1,24) = 5.4, p < 0.03. There were living with siblings x sex interactions on the scales for Acceptance of Individuation F(1,24) = 4.2, p = 0.05, and Lax Discipline, F(1,24) = 9.4, p < 0.01. On both scales, boys living with siblings had lower scores than boys not living with siblings, but the opposite pattern occurred for girls. For the divorced group, Possessiveness was the only maternal scale that was correlated with years since divorce (r a 0.38, df = 26, p < 0.05). There was also a single maternal scale that was correlated with years since father's remarriage; children gave their mothers higher Control scores as years since father's remarriage increased (r a 0.61, df = 26, p < 0.05). There were no significant correlations between any of the scales for mother and years since mother's remarriage for the divorce group (boys and girls lumped). For boys of divorce, maternal Hostile Control scores were correlated with years since divorce (r = 0.61, df = 47 13, p < 0.02). Girls showed no significant correlations between any of the maternal scales and years since divorce. There were also no correlations between the scales for mother and the years since mother's remarriage for boys or girls. Boys' scores for maternal Nonenforcement were negatively correlated with years since father's remarriage (r = 0.56, df = 13, p < 0.03). Girls' perceptions of mother showed the greatest influence of years since father's remarriage with strong correlations on three of the maternal scales. As the years since father's remarriage increased, girls saw mothers as lower on Acceptance of Individuation (r = -0.97, df = 11, p < 0.03). Girls also perceived increasing maternal Hostile Detachment as years since father's remarriage went up (r = 0.98, df = 11, p < 0.02). A near-perfect positive correlation was found between girls' perceptions of maternal Withdrawal of Relations and years since father's remarriage (r = 0.997, df = 11, p < 0.005). Effects of Demographic Variables on Children's Perceptigpg of Father Mother's and father's remarriage had no significant effects on the three factors for father. The custody arrangement affected the Firm Control factor for father. The paternal Firm Control scores were highest for children in mother custody (M = 21.5), intermediate for children in joint custody (M = 18.0), and 48 lowest for children in father custody (M = 17.1), F(2,22) = 4.9, p < 0.02. This same pattern of custody arrangement effects was seen above on the maternal Firm Control factor. In addition, there was a custody arrangement x sex interaction on the paternal Acceptance factor, F(2,22) = 7.9, p < 0.003. In mother custody, boys and girls rated father alike on Acceptance. In father and joint custody, boys viewed fathers as higher on Acceptance than did girls. Visitation by the non-custodial parent and living with siblings did not affect any of the three paternal factors. No significant main effects of mother's remarriage were seen on any of the 18 scales for father. However, there was a mother's remarriage x sex interaction on the Possessiveness scale for father, F(1,24) = 4.8, p < 0.05. If their mothers were remarried, boys gave their fathers lower Possessiveness scores than if their mothers were not remarried. In contrast, girls rated their fathers as higher on Possessiveness if their mothers were remarried. Father's remarriage was associated with significant main effects on the Rejection and Hostile Detachment scales for father. The mean Rejection score for father was higher if the father was remarried (M = 13.0) than if he was not remarried (M = 11.5), F(1,24) = 8.6, p < 0.01. The mean Hostile Detachment score for father was also higher if father was remarried (M a 14.2) than if he was not remarried (M = 11.9), F(1,24) = 14.2, p = 0.001. In addition, there were father's remarriage x sex interaction 49 effects on the paternal scales for Rejection, F(1,24) = 5.3, p = 0.03, and Hostile Detachment, F(1,24) = 5.4, p < 0.03. Girls with remarried fathers gave higher paternal Rejection and Hostile Detachment scores than girls whose fathers were not remarried. If their fathers were remarried, boys showed no effect on paternal Rejection and little effect on paternal Hostile Detachment. In summary, if father remarried, he was seen as more rejecting and as showing more hostile detachment; this was especially true for girls. There were four scales for father that were significantly affected by custody arrangement: Control (p < 0.05), Inconsistent Discipline (p < 0.005), Hostile Detachment (p < 0.03), and Extreme Autonomy (p < 0.001), (df = 2,22; see Table 10). Control scores for father were lower in joint custody than in mother or father custody. Father's Inconsistent Discipline and Hostile Detachment scores were highest in joint custody, intermediate in father custody, and lowest in mother custody. Surprisingly, fathers received the highest scores for Extreme Autonomy from children in father custody! Children in mother custody reported the lowest scores for father's Extreme Autonomy. Custody arrangement x sex interactions were found on five scales for father: Acceptance (p < 0.05), Childcenteredness (p < 0.05), Possessiveness (p < 0.03), Positive Involvement (p < 0.01), and Hostile Detachment 50 4000000 00006 4000030 002004 4000000 00000: 0.4. 0.44 0.04 0.44 4.4. 0.4. 40000000 0000000 o.o. 0.4. o.o. o.o. o.~. o.~. 00000000m no 0030000003 m.m~ o.o. 4.0. m.~. ..m. m... ucoenomuoa 000000: o.o. o.o. o... o.m. m.~. m.m. 4000xc< 0000000000 000040000H o.m. o.om o..~ o.v~ m.h. 4.4. 0c0090000n 400 m.mm m.4~ m.w~ o.m~ o.m~ m.v~ 00000300>0©0H «0 0000000000 o.4~ 0.0. o.m. o.m. 4.0. 4.m. 00000000ucwcoz o.m. o.4~ o.m. o.o~ m.v. 4.m. 0000000000 00000000000H o.m. o.m. m.~. o.m. o.m. m.4. 0000000 000000: o.o. 0.0. o.~. 0.0. 4.4. «.m. 00009 :msounu 0000000 o.om o.~m o.v. o.- 4.4. o.o. mmocm>000000H m.m~ o.om m..~ m.m~ v.4m m.m~ ucmeo>ao>sm o>0ufimom 0.4. 0.0. o.m. o.m. 4.4. 4.0. 00050000msm 0.4. o.4. o.0. o.o~ o.o~ m.m. Houucou m.4. o.o. m.~. m.~. m.~. 4... :00uowflmm o.o~ o.o. o.m. o.¢~ m.m. 4.0. mmocm>0mmommom o.w. o.om 0.0. o.- 4.m~ m.4~ mmocnmuoucwopaano m..m o.om ..mm o.m~ m.m~ m.m~ mocmumoooc 00000 040m 0000U 040m 00000 040m mamom .Honumm 00w moamom Hmmmu :o Damswmcmuu< 4000050 no muoomum .o. manna 51 (p < 0.005), (df = 2,22; see Table 10). In mother custody, boys and girls viewed father alike on these five scales. In father and joint custody, however, boys perceived father as higher on Acceptance, Childcenteredness, and Positive Involvement than girls perceived father. In addition, in father custody, boys saw fathers as higher on Possessiveness than did girls. In joint custody, girls gave fathers a much higher score on Hostile Detachment than did boys. As reported above for the maternal scales, there were no main effects due to visitation by the non-custodial parent on the paternal scales. A significant interaction effect of visitation by the non-custodial parent x sex was noted on the Hostile Detachment scale for father, F(1,24) = 4.2, p a 0.05. Boys visited by the non-custodial parent reported a higher score for father's Hostile Detachment than boys who were not visited; the reverse was true for girls. The mean paternal Inconsistent Discipline score was higher for children living with siblings (M = 16.1) than for those not living with siblings (M = 12.9), F(1,24) = 6.0, p < 0.03. There were no other effects of living with siblings on the paternal scales. There were no significant correlations for the entire divorced sample between any of the scales for father and years since divorce or years since father's remarriage. However, there was a significant correlation between one 52 paternal scale and years since mother's remarriage; as years since mother's remarriage increased, children gave their fathers lower Control through Guilt scores (r = -O.66, df = 26, p = 0.05). Boys of divorce showed no significant correlations between any paternal scales and years since divorce, years since mother's remarriage, or years since father's remarriage. In contrast, girls' views of their fathers were more influenced by the divorce variables. For girls, there was a correlation between paternal Acceptance of Individuation and years since divorce (r = 0.56, df = 11, p < 0.05). Also as years since divorce increased, girls perceived fathers as lower on Withdrawal of Relations (r = -0.63, df = 11, p < 0.03). Another negative correlation was noted for girls between paternal Nonenforcement and years since father's remarriage (r = -0.97, df = 11, p < 0.05). Again, there were no significant correlations for girls between any of the paternal scales and years since mother's remarriage. It is striking that only one scale in this entire analysis was correlated with years since mother's remarriage. In summary, the time since father's remarriage affected children's perceptions of their parents more than time since divorce or time since mother's remarriage. Also, girls' views were more greatly influenced by the time since father's remarriage than were boys' perceptions of their parents. 53 Effects of Demographic Variables on Locus of Control Within the divorced group, there were no significant main effects or interaction effects on the locus of control score for the following variables: mother's remarriage, father's remarriage, custody arrangement, visitation by the non-custodial parent, and siblings living with the child. For the total divorce sample, there were no significant correlations between locus of control and years since divorce, years since mother's remarriage, and years since father's remarriage. However, when examining the effects of the above variables by sex, girls of divorce showed a near perfect correlation (r = 0.99, df = 11, p = 0.01) between their locus of control scores and years since father's remarriage. Locus of control scores for boys of divorce were negatively correlated (r a -0.74, df = 13, p < 0.05) with years since father's remarriage. Thus, as years since father's remarriage increase girls of divorce show more external locus of control while boys of divorce show more internal locus of control. The years since divorce and years since mother's remarriage were not correlated with locus of control for boys or girls. Comparisons between the Custodial and Non-Custodial Parents The comparisons between custodial and non-custodial parents for the CRPBI factors and scales were performed via paired t-tests. These comparisons showed results similar to the mother versus father factor and scale score comparisons because 82% of children were in mother custody. 54 Girls of divorce viewed the custodial parent as significantly higher on the Firm Control factor than the non-custodial parent (t = 2.3, df = 12, p < 0.05). For boys of divorce, the custodial parent was seen as significantly higher on the Psychological Control factor than the non-custodial parent (t = 4.1, df = 14, p = 0.001). Girls of divorce saw the custodial parent as significantly higher on the Hostile Control (t = 2.2, df = 12, p = 0.05) and Instilling Persistent Anxiety (t = 2.9, df = 12, p < 0.02) scales. The exceptions to the mother vs father pattern were the differences on the Nonenforcement scale and the absence of differences on the Withdrawal of Relations scale. On the Nonenforcement scale, girls of divorce rated the custodial parent as significantly higher than the non—custodial parent (t = 2.6, df = 12, p < 0.03). Boys of divorce gave significantly higher scores to the custodial parent on Rejection (t = 3.2, df = 14, p < 0.01) and on Instilling Persistent Anxiety (t = 3.8, df = 14, p < 0.005). DISCUSSION Comparisons between Children from Divorced and Intact Families A striking result of this study was the absence of differences between children from divorced and intact families on the three CRPBI factors for mother and father. It was earlier hypothesized that children of divorce would perceive their parents as lower on Acceptance, Psychological Control, and Firm Control than children from intact families. These hypotheses were based on studies which found discipline problems and diminished parenting for children of divorce (Tooley, 1975; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1976, 1980, 1983; Hetherington, 1979). However, in the present study, overall, children of divorce do not appear to view their parents more negatively than children from intact families. Since the CRPBI factors measure gross dimensions of parent behavior, the CRPBI scales were used for more detailed comparisons. The finding of no differences between children from intact and divorced families on 17 of the 18 scales again suggests less impact of divorce on children's perceptions of parental behavior than was expected. The data also showed no significant differences in locus of control between children from divorced and intact families. This refutes the prediction that children of 5n= as .0 #83 96.33 a ‘2 ..LS 2386 APPENDIX B : Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale APPENDIX B Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control scale INSTRUCTIONS Please read each question on the following pages and answer by circling either YES or NO. Be sure to answer every question. 93 10. 11. 12. 94 Do you believe that most problems will solve themselves if you just don't fool with them? Do you believe that you can stop yourself from catching a cold? Are some kids just born lucky?‘ Most of the time do you feel that getting good grades means a great deal to you? Are you often blamed for things that just aren't your fault? Do you believe that if somebody studies hard enough he or she can pass any subject? Do you feel that most of the time it doesn't pay to try hard because things never turn out right anyway? Do you feel that if things start out well in the morning that it's going to be a good day no matter what you do? Do you feel that most of the time parents listen to what their children have to say? Do you believe that wishing can make good things happen? When you get punished, does it usually seem it's for no good reason at all? Most of the time do you find it hard to change a friend's (mind) opinion? YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 95 Do you think that cheering more than luck helps a team to win? Do you feel that it's nearly impossible to change your parent's mind about anything? Do you believe that your parents should allow you to make most of your own decisions? Do you feel that when you do something wrong there's very little you can do to make it right? Do you believe that most kids are just born good at sports? Are most of the other kids your age stronger than you are? Do you feel that one of the best ways to handle most problems is just not to think about them? Do you feel that you have a lot of choice in deciding who your friends are? If you find a four leaf clover do you believe that it might bring you good luck? Do you often feel that whether you do your homework has much to do with what kind of grades you get? Do you feel that when a kid your age decides to hit you, there's little you can do to stop him or her? Have you ever had a good luck charm? YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 96 Do you believe that whether or not people like you depends on how you act? Will your parents usually help you if you ask them to? Have you felt that when people were mean to you it was usually for no reason at all? Most of the time, do you feel that you can change what might happen tomorrow by what you do today? Do you believe that when bad things are going to happen they just are going to happen no matter what you try to do to stop them? Do you think that kids can get their own way if they just keep trying? Most of the time do you find it useless to try to get your own way at home? Do you feel that when good things happen they happen because of hard work? Do you feel that when somebody your age wants to be your enemy there's little you can do to change matters? Do you feel that it's easy to get friends to do what you want them to? Do you usually feel that you have little to say about what you get to eat at home? YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES YES NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO NO 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 97 Do you feel that when someone doesn't like you there's little you can do about it? Do you usually feel that it's almost useless to try in school because most other children are just plain smarter than you are? Are you the kind of person who believes that planning ahead makes things turn out better? Most of the time, do you feel that you have little to say about what your family decides to do? Do you think it's better to be smart than to be lucky? YES YES YES YES YES NO NO NO NO NO APPENDIX C: Letter to Parents APPENDIX C Letter to Parents Dear Parent: Our organization has been contacted by a researcher from Michigan State University's Department of Psychology who would like your help with an important project. The purpose of the project is to gain a better understanding of how children in different types of families perceive their experiences. As you know, the nature of the family and its role in society is rapidly changing. It is important for parents, teachers and others who deal with children to understand how these changes in the family affect children. This project will focus on 9 to 12 year old children. Children whose parents are still married and children whose parents are divorced will be included in the project. The project is designed to provide information which will improve understanding of children's needs. We hope that such information will be useful to parents and professionals. Participants in this project will be volunteers. If you agree to participate, you and your child will be asked to complete a few questionnaires. One parent will be asked to fill out a short family information form which takes about 10 minutes. Your child will complete two questionnaires which will require a total involvement of about 1 hour. These materials will be personally delivered to your home at your convenience. All responses will be kept strictly confidential. If you are interested in participating with your 9-12 year old child in this project, or would like further information, please return the enclosed postcard. Please check the box which indicates your interest. The director of the project will contact you personally by telephone to answer any questions and to set up an appointment time. If you do not wish to participate, please check the appropriate box and return the enclosed postcard. This is to ensure that you will not be contacted again about this project. 98 -4- 99 Thank you for your consideration of what should be a very valuable project. Sincerely, Signature of Organization Leader APPENDIX D: Parent Demographic Survey Form II. APPENDIX D PARENT DEMOGRAPHIC SURVEY FORM IDENTIFYING INFORMATION Child's name Child's sex (circle): M F Child's age Child's birthdate Mo Day Year Your name Street address City/State/Zip code Your phone number Your relationship to child PARENTS Please fill in the following information about the child's original (biological) parents: Father's name Father's age (If deceased, year of death: 19 Father's occupation Father's education Mother's name Mother's age (If deceased, year of death: 19 Mother's occupation Mother's education 100 III. 101 FAMILY STATUS (A) (B) 1. For The child currently lives with (please check one): a. both original parents (biological mother and father) b. mother only c. father only d. other (please explain) Have the children's original (biological) parents been legally divorced at any time since the child was born? Yes No If yes, please skip to Part (B). If no, please answer question 3 below. How long have the child's parents been married? (years) Please go to Part (C). children with divorced parents only When were the child's parents divorced? date (give month & year if possible) Is the child's mother currently remarried? Yes No If yes, how long has mother been remarried? (years) Is the child's father currently remarried? Yes No If yes, how long has father been remarried? (years) 102 9. Please specify the child's custody arrangement: a. mother b. father c. joint d. other 10. Does the child visit the non-custodial parent? Yes No 11. If yes, how often does the child visit the non-custodial parent? (C) Please provide the following information about the child's siblings: Number of siblings Name Age Sex Relationship with child (M or F) to child* (yes or no) * Lives please specify if sister, brother, step—sister, step— brother, half-sister, half-brother, adopted sister or brother REFERENCES ( REFERENCES Andry, R. G. Faulty paternal and maternal-child relationships, affection and delinquency. British Journal of Delinquency, 1957, Q, 34-38. Ausubel, D. P., Balthazar, E. 8., Rosenthal, I., Blackman, L. S., Schpoont, S. H., & Welkowitz, J. Perceived parent attitudes as determinants of children's ego structure. Child Development, 1954, 2;, 173-183. Battle, E. S., & Rotter, J. B. Children's feelings of personal control as related to social class and ethnic group. Journal of Personality, 1963, 11, 482-490. Beal, E. W. Children of divorce: a family systems perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 1979, ;§, 140-154- Benedek, R. 8., & Benedek, E. P. Children of divorce: can we meet their needs? Journal of Social Issues, 1979, 3;, 155-169. Berdie, K. P., & Layton, W. L. Minnesota Counseling Ipventory Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1957. Bialer, I. Conceptualization of success and failure in mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Personality, 1961, 29, 303-320. Biller, H. B. Father absence and the personality development of the male child. Developmental Bronfenbrenner, U. Some familial antecedents of responsibility and leadership in adolescents. In L. Petrullo & B. M. Bass (Eds.), Leadership and interpersonal behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1961. Bronson, W. C., Katten, E. S., & Livson, N. Patterns of authority and affection in two generations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, §§, 143-152. 103 104 Brown, A. W., Morrison, J., & Couch, G. B. Influence of affectional family relationships on character development. Journal 9; Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1947, 32, 422-428. Coleman, J. S., Campbell, E. Q., Hobson, C. J., McPartland, J., Mood, A. M., Weinfeld, F. D., & York, R. L. Eggality 9; educational opportunity, Washington, D. C.: Government Printing Office, 1966 (Superintendent of Documents, Catalog No. FS 5.238: 38001). Cooper, J. B., & Blair, M. A. Parent evaluation as a determiner of ideology. Journal 9; Genetic Psychology, 1959, 91, 93-100. Crandall, V. C., Katkovsky, W., & Crandall, V. J. Children's beliefs in their own control of reinforcements in intellectual-academic achievement situations. Child Development, 1965, 36, 91-109. Cross, H. J. College students' memories of their parents: a factor analysis of the CRPBI. Journal 9: Consulting and Clinical Psychologyp 1969, 3;, 275-278. Duke, M. D., & Lancaster, W. A note on locus of control as a function of father absence. Journal pf Genetic Psychology, 1976, 129, 335-336. Gardner, R. A. Psychotherapy with children 2; divorce. New York: Jason Aronson, Inc., 1976. Garmezy, N., Clarke, A. R., & Stockner, C. Child-rearing attitudes of mothers and fathers as reported by schizophrenic and normal patients. Journal 9; Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1961, 6;, 176-182. Glick, P. C. Children of divorced parents in demographic perspective. Journal pf Social Issues, 1979, 35, 170- 182. Goldblum, N. Changes in the the post-divorce family system and children's adjustment (Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1984). Greenfield, N. S. The relationship between recalled forms of childhood discipline and psychopathology. Journal 2; Consultinngsychology, 1959, 2;, 139-142. Herzog, 8., & Sudia, C. E. Children in fatherless families. In B. M. Caldwell & H. N. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review 9: child development research, Vol. III, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1973. 105 Hess, R. D., & Camara, K. A. Post-divorce family relationships as mediating factors in the consequences of divorce for children. Journal 9g Social Issues, 1979' 1;, 79-96. Hetherington, E. M. Divorce: a child's perspective. American Psycholo ist, 1979, 33, 851-858. Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. Divorced fathers. The Family Coordinator, 1976, 33, 417-428. Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. The aftermath of divorce. In J. H. Stevens, Jr., & M. Mathews (Eds.), Mother-child, father-child relations. Washington, D. C.: NAEYC, 1978. Hingst, A. G. Children and divorce: the child's view. Unpublished manuscript. Kalter, N. Children of divorce in an outpatient psychiatric population. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1977, 31, 40-51. Kelly, J. B., & Wallerstein, J. S. The effects of parental divorce: experiences of the child in early latency. American Journal Q§_Q£thopsychiat , 1976, 33, 20-32. Kulka, R. A., & Weingarten, H. The long-term effects of parental divorce in childhood on adult adjustment. Journal 9; Social Issues, 1979, 33, 50-78. Kurdek, L. A., & Berg, B. Correlates of children's adjustment to their parents' divorces. In L. A. Kurdek (Ed.), E2!.§$£ection§ for child development, Vol. 19, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1983. Kurdek, L. A., Blisk, D., & Siesky, A. E. Correlates of children's long-term adjustment to their parents' divorce. Developmental Psychology, 1981, 11, 565-579. Kurdek, L. A., & Siesky, A. E. Children's perceptions of their parents' divorce. Journal 9; Divorce, 1980, 3, 339-378. Lancaster, W. W., & Richmond, B. O. Perceived locus of control as a function of father absence, age, and geographic location. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1983, 133, 51-56. Levitin, T. Children of divorce: an introduction. Journal 2; Social Issues, 1979, 33, 1-23. 106 Longfellow, C. Divorce in context: its impact on children. In G. Levinger & O. C. Moles (Eds.), Divorce and sepgration: Context; causesL and consequences. New York: Basic Books, 1979. MacDonald, A. P. Internal-external locus of control. In J. R. Robinson & P. R. Schaver (Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes. Ann Arbor: Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research, 1973. McDermott, J. F. Divorce and its psychiatric sequalae in children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1970, 33, 421-427. Mitchell, K. The price tag of responsibility: a comparison of divorced and remarried mothers. Journal 9; Divorce, 1983, g, 33-42. Morrow, W. R., & Wilson, R. C. Family relations of bright high-achieving and underachieving high school boys. Child Development, 1961, 33, 501-510. Nowicki, S., & Roundtree, J. Correlates of locus of control in a secondary school population. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 5, 477-478. Nowicki, S., & Strickland, B. R. A locus of control scale for children. Journal 9; Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973, 33, 148-154. Parish, T. S., & Nunn, G. D. Locus of control as a function of family type and age at onset of father absence. The Journal 9; Psychology, 1983, 113, 187- 190. Reinhard, D. W. The reaction of adolescent boys and girls to the divorce of their parents. Journal o; Clinical Child Psychology, 1977, g, 21-23. Renson, G. J., Schaefer, E. S., & Levy, B. I. Cross- national validity of a spherical conceptual model for parent behavior. Child Development, 1968, 33, 1229- 1235. Rode, A. Perceptions of parental behavior among alienated adolescents. Adolescence, 1971, g, 19-38. Rohrlich, J. A., Ranier, R., Berg-Cross, L., & Berg-Cross, G. The effects of divorce: a research review with a developmental perspective. Journal 9; Clinical Child Psychology, 1977, g, 15-20. 107 Rosen, R. Children of divorce: what they feel about access and other aspects of the divorce experience. Journal 23 Clinical Child Psychology, 1977, 3, 24-27. Rotter, J. B. Social learning and clinical psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1954. Rotter, J. B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 1966, 33, (1, Whole No. 609). Santrock, J. W., & Warshak, R. A. Father custody and social development in boys and girls. Journal 93 Social Issues, 1979, 33, 112-125. Schaefer, E. S. Children's reports of parental behavior: an inventory. Child Development, 1965, 33, 413-424. (a) Schaefer, E. S. A configurational analysis of children's reports of parent behavior. Journal 93 Consulting Psycholquyygg, 552-557. (b) Schludermann, E., & Schludermann, S. Replicability of factors in children's report of parent behavior (CRPBI). The Journal 93 Psychology, 1970, 33, 239- 249. Schludermann, E., & Schludermann, S. Adolescent perception of parent behavior (CRPBI) in Hutterite communal society. The Journal 23 Psychology, 1971, 33, 29-39. Schludermann, S., & Schludermann, E. Sociocultural change and adolescents' perceptions of parent behavior. Developmental Psychology, 1983, 13, 674-685. Selman, R. L., & Byrne, D. F. A structural-developmental analysis of levels of role taking in middle childhood. Child Develgpment, 1974, 33, 803-806. Serot, N. M., & Teevan, R. C. Perception of the parent- child relationship and its relation to child adjustment. Child Development, 1961, 33, 373-378. Slater, E. J., Stewart, K. J., & Linn, M. W. The effects of family disruption on adolescent males and females. Adolescence, 1983, 13, 931-942. Tooley, K. Antisocial behavior and social alienation post divorce: the "man of the house" and his mother. American Journal 23 Orthopsychiatry, 1976, 33, 33-42. 108 U. S. Bureau of the Census. Mgrital status and family status: March 1980. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1981. Wallerstein, J. S. Children of divorce: the psychological tasks of the child. American Journal 93 Orthopsychiatry, 1983, 33, 230-243. Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. The effects of parental divorce: the adolescent experience. In E. J. Anthony & C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child 33 his family: children 33 psychiatric risk, Vol. 3, New York: Wiley & Sons, 1974. Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. The effects of parental divorce: experiences of the preschool child. Journal 23 the American Academy 93 Child Psychiatry, 1975, 13, 600-616. Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. The effects of parental divorce: experiences of the child in later latency. American Journal 93 Orthopsychiatry, 1976, 33, 256- 269. Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. Divorce counseling: a community service for families in the midst of divorce. American Journal 93 Orthopsychiatry, 1977, ‘33, (1), 4-22. Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. Surviving the breakup: how children and parents cope with divorce. New York: Basic Books, 1980. Warshak, R. A., & Santrock, J. W. The impact of divorce in father-custody and mother-custody homes: the child's perspective. In L. A. Kurdek (Ed.), New directiops 323 child development, Vol. 19, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1983. Weiss, R. Growing up a little faster: the experience of growing up in a single-parent household. Journal g3 Social Issues, 1979, 33, 97-111. Westman, J. C., Cline, D. W., Swift, W. J., & Kramer, D. A. Role of child psychiatry in divorce. Archives 93 General Psychiatry, 33, 416-420. urn 10.1)" .I? .00). 1 x "11111111171111111111167)“