.___‘ -u------ V’lSlTlNG FOREIGN SCIENTISTS IN THE UNITEB STATES! THE IMPACT OF SYSTEMIC AND ROLE CIRCUMSCREPUON AND DESSOCEATIVE EXPERLENCES ON THE HOHOGENIETY OF THE INTERM'FMML WHERE". COEKUNITY Thests for “n 0:»th of pit. D. MECHIGAN STATE UNIVERSHY Christopher K. Vanderpool 1971 L. I __ —. t’i (/4 5 £2 13, is? “a 1 MMMBM . K/ t. .5.”,m,. ., 3129301068124 LI}; {A R Y 2 Michigan State ““ University This is to certify that the thesis entitled Visiting Foreign Scientists in the United States:' The Impact of Systemic and Role Circumscription And Dissociative Experiences on the Homogeneity 0f the International Scientific Community. presented by Christopher K. Vanderpool has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein Sociology . . v , ((76 AM/ Z / (327471 ; 2 ‘VJ Major professor Date January 26, 1971 0-7839 "—- -—~—4——-—r—-..-—". Ma ti.) “no: Y 2 . A .55 bud rod \vq own-Ar. L‘J J‘Lbbx. nv-L': " Ho soc-J ' Au‘ 6 V4591 : “A'V'Vy-i L. 'U*--“.. y t I .. c.9251 a: C‘JZSQV of n4. \r“ a 0. -2 ea ABSTRACT VISITING FOREIGN SCIENTISTS IN THE UNITED STATES: THE IMPACT OF SYSTEMIC AND ROLE CIRCUMSCRIPTION AND DISSOCIATIVE EXPERIENCES ON THE HOMOGENEITY OF THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY BY Christopher K. Vanderpool Prior studies in the sociology of science have focused on the internal structure of the scientific community and have only briefly touched upon the so- cietal and transcultural dimensions of science. More- over, these research endeavors have stressed that science is essentially a homogeneous social structure in which there is a uniformity of behavioral and atti- tudinal patterns practiced by scientists throughout the world. This dissertation, incontradistinction, attempts to locate variations in the behavior and attitudes of scientists. It examines: 1)the impact of systemic cir- cumscription, as evidenced in the level of development of educational and scientific institutions in a series of nations, and role circumscription, as evidenced in the types of work roles previously performed by scien- tists, and 2)the exposure of scientists to dissociative experiences, the movement of scientists from one social system to another or their contact with the members of an exogenous social system. These three are related to the homogeneity of the international scientific community Christopher K. Vanderpool as evidenced in this sample of a population of foreign scientists in one part of the United States. Visiting foreign scientists in the United States compose the population in this study. Using the C.I.C. (Committee on Institutional Cooperation) universities* as a site for the research, eighty-two interviews were conducted using an open-ended interview schedule. A questionnaire was constructed composed of fixed alter- native questions and sent to scientists in several universities in the Midwestern region. One hundred and forty questionnaires were returned. The total N of the study, then,is two hundred and twentyftwo. Because a random sampling technique was not employed, Yule's Q, contigency coefficients, and a comparison of percentage differences are used in analyzing the results. The data gathered indicate that systemic and role circumscription are related to each other and to expo- sure to dissociative experiences, societal social re- sponsibility, to professional participation, and to seeing differences between the social systems of work in the United States and the home country. Their rela- tionship to scientific social responsibility and non- professional participation are, however, either incon- clusive or weak. It is also shown that the greater the * The C.I.C. universities are the Universities of Wisconsin, Illinois, Indiana, Minnesota, Michigan, Chicago, Iowa, and Michigan State, Ohio State, Purdue, and Northwestern universities. ““5 a Unl- 'v u... U --.. Christopher K. Vanderpool degree of role circumscription, the greater the degree of exposure to dissociative experiences. This result suggests that scientists from developing nations typically go out of their home country to make themselves visible to their colleagues abroad, to get information on current developments in their fields, and to establish inter- personal colleagial relationships with other scientists in their field of work. Scientists from developing nations and those scien- tists who perform teaching, administrative, or consul- ting roles in their countries of origin are more likely to view the work situation in the United States as dif- ferent from the social system of work in their home countries than scientists from developed nations and those who perform research, teaching-research, profes- sional, or publication roles. As a consequence, the former scientists have to redefine their roles and social identities in the social system of work in the united States. Such a role redefinition and a trans- formation of social identities is less likely to occur amongst the latter scientists. Furthermore, scientists from developing nations usually rank their home countries as a peripheral area of scientificactivity in a ranking of nations in their fields. Scientists from developed nations, on the other hand, view their home countries as a center or leader in their fields. Data are presented which indicate that Biter‘ie a unite to 33:9 ’ .‘t‘in u 4..., , 5 Christopher K. Vanderpool those scientists from the periphery are more likely to hold non-mainline positions (research assistant or as- sociate) than mainline positions (instructor to full professor) in the United States. The opposite is true for scientists from developed nations in the centers of their fields. Moreover, the scientists from devel— oping nations are more likely to consider the systemic linkage networks of exchanges of students, resources, and information between their home countries and one of the centers in their fields, the United States, as being non-reciprocal than scientists from developed nations who view these networks as reciprocal. The results also show that exposure to dissociative experiences fosters post-modernity, worldmindedness, and a universalistic orientation to social interaction and, to some degree, to future work location. But its effect on third cultural network involvement and a universalis- tic orientation to living location is mixed. Educational and prior work experiences abroad are negatively corre— lated with third cultural network involvement. Making trips to foreign countries in the status of a profes- sional scientists, however, stimulates such contact. Exposure to dissociative experiences is related to a particularistic, rather than universalistic, orientation to living location. This finding suggests an evaluative ability. By becoming aware of alternatives to their society and culture, the majority of scientists make a Christopher K. Vanderpool preference for their home country or another country, rather than having no preference. Finally, the findings reveal that the majority in this study share thte,same beliefs, perspectives and orientations and behavs’in a similar fashion both in and outside of the scientific community. This homogeneity of responses give credence tdpthe idea of an interna- tional scientific community, at least with regard to these scientists. Science, therefore, is not a hetero— geneous collectivity. Such a uniformity of behavior and attitudes could only emerge out of a common social structure and normative structure that transcends tradi- tional societal and cultural differences. Science can be considered, then, as a thirdfioulture. «v‘ '1’: .5. I‘d RY: nvcv - 05.4.!- G can-l ..\,,l ,s... "A‘ n. .- VISITING FOREIGN SCIENTISTS IN THE UNITED STATES: THE IMPACT OF SYSTEMIC AND ROLE CIRCUMSCRIPTION AND DISSOCIATIVE EXPERIENCES ON THE HOMOGENEITY OF THE INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY By ‘_ Christopher Ki Vanderpool A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Sociology ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS A dissertation marks not only the culmination of a research project which is hopefully a contribution to an academic discipline, but also signifies the end of a graduate education and the beginning of a pro- fessional career. Several individuals have contributed greatly to my fulfillment of these tasks: 1)the members of my committee-_Professors William Form, Charles Loomis, Frederick waisanen, and John Useem; 2)a silent member of my committee-—Professor Ruth Hill Useem; 3)my col- league and co-researcher——Sal P. Restivo; 4)several fellow graduate students at Michigan State University-- Florence McCarthy, Arnold Holden, Susan Asch, Shelby Stweman, Robert Shelly, Anne McMahon, and Michael Loukinen; and 5)my wife and son--Lorraine and Aaron Vanderpool. William Form has been throughout my sociological career a friend, a teacher, and a colleague. He a- wakened in me and encouraged a strong appreciation of the various facets of social structure and macro- sociology and their ramifications on society and soc— iology. In many ways, Professor Form is a "sociological father" to a whole cohort of graduate students in soc- iology and I hope he will be proud of his "sons and ii iii daughters" achievements. Charles Loomis first acquainted me with the con- cept of systemic linkage and this dissertation re— lies heavily on this important concept both explicitly and implicitly. In several reading courses, Pro- fessor Loomis allowed me to open up "Pandora's Box” in sociological theory stimulating and encouraging my interest in political sociology and classical and contemporary’theory. Frederick waisanen, as this dissertation in— dicates, has had an important affect on my appreciation of comparative sociology and social psychology. He was always Willing to sit down with me to pound out ideas and to criticize my fledgling knowledge of social psycholOgy and social change. As a result of his seminars, his colleagueship and friendship, Professor Waisanen has been a "Dissociative experience" in my graduate training. John Useem, chairman of my doctoral committee, has provided the guidance, understanding, and crit— icism which is necessary in making a graduate ed- ucation an intellectual and human experience. Always encouraging a full exploration of a problem, a search of the literature of sociology and allied areas of inquiry, and a macroscopic analysis, Professor Useem ‘o k|‘ (I) AA?.:’\‘ ~ '---."JI , . Mun: no . via .a cw; Ivy-3n {ION-1:7. n - 1a , no“. a a“ '1‘... "'l l.‘_‘ 3 , . .r "...c, I R‘. “~p i.::~”’a‘ CI) 5“ g. .1 l v has stimulated what was once a dormant sociological imagination. He was the first to acquaint me with the areas of the sociology of science and comparative soc- iology and the ways in which cross—societal and cultural contact give rise to "third cultures." The untold hours he has spent With me on this research project and others have extended far beyond the usual teacher— student relationship. Professor RUth Hill Useem sat in on many of these after hours seminars. Many of the points she raised in these discussions have been incorporated in this dissertation. Her criticisms and assistance have in many ways made this research endeavor possible. Sal P. Restivo has been my collaborator on this study and a prior one. Through our many intellectual battles and cooperative activities, we have established a collegial and friendship relationship which will con- tinue in our future professional careers. My fellow graduate students at Michigan State have also been a great assistance in the completion of my doctorate education. Susan Asch and Florence McCarthy tempered many of my crude ideas in the seminars held at the home of the Useems. Arnold Holden ex- plored with me the issues raised by Professor waisanen's model of modernization and several times provided help O ‘ A? N y‘a'aiu.’ |1 me43964 [‘7 | V‘Ahbufi vbv~ . uvfi‘l A an. VP‘ 5.... «AS .1" A.~ "W “Va U'Iye 5..“ ”4 Va ,. «ea; v in the statistical analysis. Michael Loukinen, Anne McMahon, Robert Shelly and Shelby Stewman gave me soc— ial and intellectual support throughout these last five years. I would like to also thank the National Science Foundation, the National Institute of Mental Health and the Hazen Foundation which has supported this study financially. NSF provided a dissertation research grant to cover expenses occured in the completion of this research project. NIMH awarded a fellowship which allowed me freedom from other tasks while completing this study. The Hazen Foundation provided support for many of secretarial expenses entailed in this report and enabled comparative studies directly related to this dissertation to be undertaken. Finally, but not least, I am very grateful to my wife and son for their understanding, patience and love. Lorraine sacrificed much of her little spare time to type and criticize numerous drafts of research proposals and reports. We have traveled many social miles together from the streets of the inner city of Chicago to our current milieu. My family and I have found sustenance and consciousness in this journey. 6 d n§b .iu a. h. t . i .1 9 M». ”H Rd u“ 3 a. I. a. a. 2. ‘II ad. 5. av Y... 13...; Table of Contents Page Iii-St Of TableSOOOOOOOOO0.00.0.0....000.000...OViii LiSt 0f FigureSOOOOOOOO0.00000000000000000.000 Xiv Chapter 1: Explorations of the Sources of Homogeneity and Heterogeneity in the SCientiin community.oooooooooooooooooooooooo° 1 General Statement of the Problem......... 5 Specification of Variables............... 13 Dissociative Experiences................. 22 Additional Areas of Investigation........ 36 Research Design.......................... 39 Chapter 2: General Characteristics of the Respondents and The Impact of Systemic and Role Circumscription on Dissociative EXPerienceS.OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.00.0...0.0.0o 51 Systemic Circumscription................. 52 Role Circumscription..................... 54 Dissociative Experiences................. 57 Systemic Circumscription and Role Cir- cumscription vs. Dissociative Experiences. 67 Additional Characteristics and Background Of the Respondents.0.0000000000000000.COOC 7"" Chapter 3: Center and Periphery in Science..... 89 Chapter A: Homogeneity and Heterogeneity in the SCientifiC communitYooooooooooooooooooooooo109 Third Cultural Networks...................110 Post-Modern Orientation...................118 Universalistic Orientations to Work, Social Interaction, and Living Location...124 vi a. I ’V“ DJV- " Q WV" A. J. n v2“. 0 "o i q. . rO“," ” J. bye. ~ - . Q I " T uév'Eean U Q u R v“p’3 \' UMAUVJ “i vii worldmindedneSSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO.132 Social and Scientific Responsibility.....138 Professional and Non-professional PartiCipationcoOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0011+? Chapter 4: Further Exploration: Differences Between Social Systems of Work in the United States and the Home Country............153 Differences Between Students.............155 Differences in Authority Relations.......168 Differences Between Colleagues...........174 Differences in the Types of Work Roles PerformedOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO.o184 Further Exploration in Differences Between Social Systems of Work...........196 Chapter 5: Summary of Results and Conclusions.203 Systemic and Role Circumscription........205 Dissociative Experiences.................214 Homogeneity in Science...................217 Appendix A: Interview Schedule and Questions naireOOOOOOOCCOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO220 Appendix B: Supplementary Tables..............274 Bibliography.ooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo277 {rm :1. M» N... M“ Nu. m... “a. MC Av u a. n... .w.|n .I... . . .. .IN. . I . . L .m; 1” .nv. Cu u: S.«J I. 3“ s D“ D” Ah 0-. .r. C C C s .I U, in" lo 2 3 I“ 5 5 71 ms” 0 Q . . . . . . . . . . . . o . . . . . . . . . . . . C U . . . . . 0 I I \ a O Q 9 \. . . . . . . . . . u o s u - - . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 . C. Wu Dual. Pu t FOIL; ,r. Table 10 11 viii List of Tables Initial Sampling Design: Level of Development and Type of Science at Three Midwestern Universities........... Characteristics of Respondents: Type of Science and Level of Development of the Home CountryOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Systemic Circumscription: Level of Development Of NationSooooooooooooooooco Percentage Distribution of Degree of Role Circumscription: Rank Order of the Performance of Scientific Work Roles in the Home Country............... Role Circumscription in the Home Country: Contigency Coefficient Analysis of Rank Orders of the Per- formance of Scientific Work Boles....... Percentage Distribution and Correlation of the Degree of Role Circumscription of the Respondents in their Country of Origin by their Degree of Systemic Cir- scriptionOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.0I... Percentage Distribution of Level of Exposure to Varying Types of Disso- ciative Experiences..................... Correlational Matrix of Systemic and Role Circumscription and Dissociative ExperienceSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Correlations Between Role Circumscrip- tion and Dissociative Experiences: Controlling for Systemic Circumscrip- tionOO0.0.000...OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Age Distribution of the Respondents..... Percentage Distribution of Years in Which the Doctorate was Received by the Respondents.OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Page 42 47 52 54 55 56 62 65 69 71+ 75 """""""" ..... ‘ | ' I o a """" 33333 ooooo Q A u I . ' . I Q 1 fl . , Title .4 \J‘ p _A (—1.) 19 23 Ix) *J yer. orffini :“‘¢“ ne- den.s. O Table 12 13 in 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 ix Page Percentage Distribution of Length of Years Between Bachelor's Degree and the Highest Degree Received by the Respondents............................. 77 Systemic Circumscription and the Level of Development of the Country in Which the Bachelor's Degree was Received by the RespondentSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.0.0. 78 Systemic Circumscription and the Level of Development of the Country in Which the Master's Degree was Received by the Respondents......................... 80 Systemic Circumscription and the Level of Development of the Country in Which the Doctorate was Received by the Re— spondentSOOOO.0.0.0.0....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 8O Occupations of the Fathers, Mothers, and Spouses of the Respondents in Science and Other Academic Fields and in Non-Scientific and Non-Academic Occupations............................. 85 Percentage Distribution of the Type of Organizations Which Employed the Respon- dents.OOOCOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 86 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Perceived Position of the Home Country in Relationship to the Leading Coun- tries in the Respondents' Fields........ 92 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Perceived Future Mobility of the Home Country in Relationship to the Leading Countries in the Respondents' Fields.... 94 Systemic and Role Circumscription and the Position of the Respondents in the Social System of Work in the United States: Mainline vs. Non—Mainline Po- SitionSOOOOOOOOO0.0000000000000000000.00 98 Systemic and Role Circumscription and the Type of the Type of Exchanges Be— tween the United States and the Home Countries of the Respondents: Recipro- cal and Non-Reciprocal Exchanges........ 103 o o o , o i l 1 o I c u n a o o o u 0 i i C O I o O I o O I o v 5 a s O O D I o o 9 o g 0 o O O Q I O O O O O O C O I ‘ Q I 't O D I I O O O 0 O U 0 O O 0 Q O O O O O O 0 oust-vov-QOOOOOIOOOOOCO. 27 30 K» ’ r: scoriefi f‘ fin .orre-s ces to V “'A?‘" I‘eUnVA .’. Eercent tire C r Post-E: H v.4 b. I‘L Table 22 23 2A 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 3% Percentage Distribution of Involvement in Third Cultural Networks by the Re— spondentSOOOOOOOO0.0.0.0....000.00.00.00 Correlations of Dissociative Experien- ces to Involvement in Third Cultural Networks..0...OOOOOOOOOOOOCOCOOOOOOOOO.O Percentage Distribution of Type of Fu— ture Orientation of the Respondents: Post-Modern and Non-Post-Modern Orien- tationSooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo Dissociative Experiences vs. Indices of Post-Modern Orientation................. Percentage Distribution of Respondents' Type of Orientation to Work, Social In— teraction, and Living Location: Univer- salism and Particularism................ Dissociative Experiences vs. Universa— listic Orientation to Work, Social In- teraction, and Living Location.......... Percentage Distribution of the Variables Affecting the Choice of a Work Location and Their Level of Importance........... Percentage Distribution of World View Change: EffeCtooooooooooooooooooooooocoo Percentage Distribution of World View Change: Referent........................ Percentage Distribution of World View Change: DireCtionOOOOOOOOOOOO00.00.00... Dissociative Experiences vs. Worldmind— edness: World View Change Effect, Re- ferent, and DireCtionooooooooooooooooooo Percentage Distribution of the Indices of Social Responsibility at the Socie- tal Level and Type of Responsibility.... Percentage Distribution of Social Re- sponsibility at the Scientific Level: Obligation to Next Generation of Scien- tiStSOOOOOOOO...0.0.0000...OOOOOOOOOOOOO Page 113 114 121 122 126 126 131 136 136 136 138 140 142 v s 9 v u I - - . > o I . . JGUL'J.- ‘ Ee"ce:t A \g.) \)\ ... .. ... .. ... .. snonsit Level bless r of Res. .7 -\ 33 :erce:* 0 3‘ 0‘ N 0rd SFSte: 39 Percer Profs: Atten: tior ‘__. (-J .‘f’ m‘ f. D I ‘41 3.. l ‘ I I 9 t O O t I of coca... 42 S‘J‘. “3 Svs xi Table Page 35 Percentage Distribution of Social Re- sponsibility at the Scientific Level: National and International Obligation To Next Generation of Scientists........ 142 36 Percentage Distribution of Social Re- sponsibility at the Scientific Level: Level of Importance of Scientific Pro— blems as a Factor in Influencing Choice Of ResearCh TOPICSoooooooooooooooooooooo 1&3 37 Systemic and Role Circumscription vs. Societal and Scientific Social Respon— 81bility0000000000.0.0.0....00......O... 141+ 38 Percentage Distribution and Correlations of Non-Professional Participation to Systemic and Role Circumscription....... 149 39 Percentage Distribution of Level of Professional Participation Indices: Attendance at Meetings, Book Publica- tion, Paper Publication................. 150 40 Systemic Circumscription vs. Profession- al PartiCipationoooooooooooooooooooooooo 151 41 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences Between the Social Systems of Work in the Home Country and the united States: Differences in Students.. 156 42 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences Between American and Home Country Students: Differences Based on Collegial Professor-Student Relation— ShipSOO00......OOOOCOOOOOOOOOOOCOOOOOOOO 158 43 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences Between American and Home Country Students: Differences Based on Breadth of Knowledge of Field of Stu- dentSOOOOOOOOOCOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0000......O 159 44 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences Between American and Home Country Students: Differences Based on Work Habits of Students................. 160 tttttt . ............... e U I Q..- oooooo I no 0-1 s v Q S 39‘ ....... Ol‘ 9' ..... ooooo (§.\ 47 \J“ (‘5 N 53 Svsteri the Per the So: “nuv‘”fil Jul-Lo U. " ences i Author: tion... (f C) U) :_3' H"‘3 (b ' ‘a m "*J ( o— ,: (D (D 24 ‘1 E l A A a?\ b 535’ :i (‘D t (b Table 45 46 47 48 49 5O 51 52 53 54 xii Systemic and Role Circumscription and the Perception of Differences Between the Social Systems of Work in the Home Country and the United States: Differ- ences in Relations with Persons in Authority Positions in the Work Situa— tionOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO... Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences of Authority Relations in the United States and the Home Country: Degree of Collegiality of Relationships with Persons in Authority Positions in the work SituationOOOOOOO000.00.00.00... Percentage Distribution of Type of Agreement with Selected Differences of American and Home Country Colleagues.... Correlations Between Systemic and Role Circumscription and Selected Differences of American and Home Country Colleagues. Systemic and Role Circumscription and Type of Work Role Which is Central to the Scientists' Fields in the Home countryOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOC0.0000000000000. Systemic and Role Circumscription and Type of Work Role Which is Central to the Scientists' Fields in the United StateSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOCOO Systemic and Role Circumscription and Type of Work Role Performed in the Uni- ted StateSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Role Circumscription and Presence or Absence of Changes in the Type of Work Roles Performed in the United States as Compared to Those Performed in the Home countrYOOOOOOO0.0.0.000....00....0...... Percentage Distribution of Types of Differences in Work Involvement in the United States as Compared to the Home Country and the Acknowledgement of Dif- ferences in Work Involvement............ Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences in Work Involvement in the United States as Compared to the Home Page 169 171 178 180 185 186 188 189 192 CountryOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.00.0.0.0...0..COO193-“ e ”M .mu. 9... (K H Ccrre ES 5 b Sta C u of Dis 1 Table 55 xiii Correlations of Differences Between Social Systems of Work in the United States and theHome Country and Type of Dissociative Experience: Differen- ces Between Students and in Relations with Persons in Authority Positions..... Appendix B Number of Non-Respondents to the Questionnaire: Type of Science and InstitutionOOOOOOOOOOOOOO000.000.00.00O. Percentage Distribution of Respondents by Their Country of Origin.............. Page 198 275 276 xiv List of Figures Figure Page 1 The Points of Reference and Foci of the Normative Structure of SCienceOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.0.0000...O. 7 Exploration Ml :p‘pup 0") ‘un “v «V. v L v Chapter 1 Explorations of the Sources Homogeneity and Hetereogeneity in the Scientific Community social ins ti 3 5.. vevfien scio 3: a SOCietV'r One of the present general goals of in- vestigations in the area of the sociology of science is the development of concepts, pro- positions, and models sufficiently abstract to permit the comparison of different cultures and societies in terms of an analysis of their social institutions (De Gre, 1955: 6). This process of conceptualization and model-building must, how- ever, remain sufficiently concrete to enable an exploration of the historical and ecological dimensions of the emergence of science as a social institution and of the systemic linkages between science and the other social institutions of a society. Since science is by nature inter- national (Einstein, 1950; Gilpin, 1968; Parthasarathi, 1967), research and theorizing in the sociology of science provides an adequate context for analyzing the normative and behavioral patterns which emerge in the process of scientists interacting with one another across national, societal,and cultural boundaries (Useem and Useem, 1968). Hence,the study of the institution of science and its members can yield information about such general social phe- nomena as the process of institutionalization and institution-building in varying societal settings, the growth of a rational ethos throughout the world, and the structure and development of international | 33:2'inities . £11 in exploré the interrela‘. the values an: a variety of 2 ”Isis iie: have-var, has 2 research lite: inquiry has p: ture of the s tented upon 3.’ science. t. . he: rigorous 3 communities. Substantively such studies can also be use— ful in explorations of the structure of science, the interrelationship of science and society, and the values and activities shared by scientists from a variety of societies and cultures. This ideal goal of the sociOlogy of science, however, has not been accomplished. Theoretical enui research literature in this area of sociological inquiry has primarily focused on the internal struc- ture of the scientific community and has only briefly touched upon the societal and transcultural dimensions of science. The "autonomous” normative structure has been rigorously defined, elaborated upon, and reform- ulated in light of existing social theory and re- search by Merton (1938 and 1957), Parsons (1951), Barber (1952) and Storer (1966). But the delimitation of the systemic linkages between science and society and the effects of these relations on the structure of the scientific institution, its members, and socie- ties have not been theoretically or substantively articulated in a comparative perspective. Studies in the united States on the effect of scientific breakthroughs on society (Price, 1963) and the accommodation of scientists to industrial and governmental bureaucracies (Corwin, 1966; Kornhauser, 1962; Pelz and Andrews, 1967; Strauss and Rainwater, 1962 ) have been the primary research *‘rms which 1A paw—V 9 O »:"H . u suglb‘on’ \ .: :& mantles or. .. I ' . :csmies hav» C"DI'\ *n ’9‘va rr;a \L‘ ) X‘yg‘{on 1 ‘1 .5. a v‘ U. the aiiustmer. & .he sciences ' , 3:1 & {fists ‘11 ts. fists. 2: aftttif 0 c6 thigfie‘SOCial 13‘3‘3 ypof‘the 11M esp/Casi SHEA 3‘1 be‘ We}: not or 4 themes which have touched upon this domain of inquiry. In addition, the impact of governmental policies and priorities on science in the United States and other countries have been explored extensively by Dupree (1957), Brooks (1968), Price (1967) and Shils (1968). There also have been studies of scientific manpower in other nations and the migration of scientists from peripheral sectors of the international scien- tific community to the centers of the scientific enterprise (Beijer, 1969; Committee on International Migration, 1970; Harbison and Myers, 1964). Finally, the adjustment of foreign students and scholars in the sciences to theitest and back home and to idea systems generated in thetiest has been extensively explored by Useem and Useem (1955), Kroche (1958) and Swisher (1958). But the values, behavior, and roles which link them to segments of their society and to a series of international communities have yet to be sociologically studied.1 This study of visiting foreign scientists in the united States attempts to partially fill this 1 Hagstrom's study of the values and roles of 76 scien- tists did touch upon this area in an examination of the extent and operation of social control within the scientific community (1965). However, his analysis of the social influences in the scientific community which produce conformity to scientific values and norms emphasizes the internal dimensions of the be- liefs and behavior of scientists. This study is con- cérned not only with this aspect of the social life of scientists, but also with cognitive and behavioral di- mensions of scientific behavior in areas external to science. vaii 1‘? f 09115: *‘cenonena: 1)* Lists of thei! 4.- to‘reip natit world communi‘ ceriences in both here in or these expe Tithe signifi of new social tions. This ' : szrlrsboard . positions wh; behavior and CCitexts. Zeneral Stat- S-d theoriz 1;. 31:1 gilded t‘ EUROSEHEO‘QS O O 1 ~ On tWoi 5 void by focusing its attention on three interrelated phenomena: 1)the perspectives shared by foreign scien— tists of their function in segments of their society, in their national scientific communities and in the world community of scientists; 2)their actual ex- periences in the performance of scientific work roles both here in the united States and abroad and the impact of these experiences on their values and behavior; and 3)the significance of their participation in the creation of new social patterns in developed and developing na- tions. This investigation will hopefully provide a springboard for the development of concepts and pro- positions which will allow future comparisons of the behavior and values of scientists in varying societal contexts. General Statement of the Problem. Current research and theorizing in the sociology of science is stimulated and guided by an underlying proposition: science, con- sidered as an institution, estate, or community is homogeneous in nature. Homogeneity is seen as exist- ing on two interdependent levels, structural and norm- ative. Structural uniformity in science can be char- acterized as a set of patterns of interaction, roles and status positions centrally organized about the maintenance, transmission, extension, or application settings: 1).. satians ulcer sional organ' fornals, ass tenational c le-ige is sitar zetworks of s tific fields where, as in irrational . DCience Fonti HYDRA \ h-Qvo, etc.’ {,2 “it‘d t0 fu: tfiadvisorv g '0‘!!- ‘ W“H.011 to EC“: i. .. (4.034). m1 of 30'“ + ”194'; 0‘2: : .3‘I‘iza I t1012; \I 6 of knowledge (Storer, 1966: 17; 75). This social syste: manifests itself primarily in the following settings: 1)workshops, laboratories, and field stations where knowledge is produced; 2)profes- sional organizations and their various organs (e.g., journals, associational meetings, committees, in- ternational conferences, etc.) through which know- ledge is shared, exchanged, and evaluated; 3)small networks of scientists in subdisciplines of scien- tific fields (e.g., ring theorists in mathematics) where, as in professional organizations, knowledge is transmitted and evaluated; 4)national and in- ternational governmental agencies (e.g., National Science Foundation, National Institute of Health, UNESCO, etc.) from whibh financial support for the creation and application of knowledge is dissem— inated to future generations of knowledge creators; 6)advisory groups which provide scientific in- formation to decision makers in the political, economic, military and numerous other social sectors of society; and 7)universities, industries and other organizations where men perform research tasks as well as other activities. 2Social system can be defined as the patterned in- teraction of social actors "...whose relations to each other are mutually oriented through the defini- tion and mediation of a pattern of structured and shzred symbols and expectations." (Loomis and Loomis, 19 5: 2 . We f’mCtiO-ei yank ' 1 'r’; "a ro.e be. a teractional r '=.1‘ r I Q ' vivaceo-AAOJS svsten of “or AA 0' a. I :r.a.e besavi I e‘ A‘\ ENS 0* .13“! o ‘ . a. . In each of these loci of activities relevant to the functioning of science as an ongoing social process, the role behavior of scientists in a series of in- teractional networks and statuses is governed by the ”autonomous" normative structure of science. This system of norms delimits the boundaries of appro- priate behavior with regard to knowledge cultivation (Merton, 1965: 113-114). Moreover, these norms speci- fy the proper orientation and action of scientists to the body of scientific knowledge, interaction patterns among scientists, and their personal pys- chological state (see Figure 1). If deviations from these norms occur, the integrity of science's native commodity, objective information, is endangered (Storer, 1966: 39). According to this perspective, Figure 1: The Points of Reference and Foci of the Normative Structure of Science* Point of Reference: The Body of Interaction The Scientists Focus of Scientific Among Psychological Norm: Knowledge Scientists State Orient- -- Organized EmotionaIIi ation Objectivity Scepticism Neutrality Action General- Disinterest- ization Communality edness *Storer, 1966: 81. scientists mus are exte..sive prise is (lest: science :anif. tists to a se the. in the s 0H t I ....S COR. 3PT. SFStei of sci Several i“): assume”. 1:5319V‘Ae. V0. V A {1:1 to the E (D d (D 3 — ‘i . "2;. 5 $07310. 14,. * “‘~.{ 8 scientists must conform to these norms, for if there are extensive violations of them the scientific enter- prise is destroyed. Thus, normative homogeneity of science manifests itself in the conformity of scien- tists to a set of norms controlling their participa— tion in the scientific community and, in response to this conformity, specifying the nature of the social system of science. Several disturbing consequences have followed 'from assuming structural and normative homogeneity in science. First, relatively little attention is paid to the external linkages of science and an over- emphasis is placed on the internal structure of science. Analyses of the systemic linkages of science to other sectors of society have focused on those interrelation- ships which contribute to the maintenance of science as an ”autonomous” social structure. Policy studies by sociologists, political scientists and others have examined the resource linkages between science and government, specifying what impact, if any, these relations will have on the interacting parties. Some strides in explicating these reciprocal effects have been made by Shils (1968), Brooks (1968), Price (1967), Greenberg (1967), and others. Their efforts, how- ever, for the most part have been speculative, sensi- tizing sociologists to the types of variations systemic linkages may produce in science, without verifying these 5 mi" Effec-IS e2.” “ iii values he If variations licensiois of ative system these norms e eie on sole: of scientist In the of conical: lire under research (3.; the strivinr ‘ '93 0f scier.l . ) yPONOti: 1’ fat ‘ 2.0 O .orces sole ‘i “‘1 the mom: < C (D ’1 c+ :J‘ (D H- . ,3 ‘ —-—- — __ effects empirically. Secondly, the behavioral patterns enacted by scientists within and outside of the scientific com- munity, their function in varying societal contexts, and values held by them are assumed to be similar. If variations in the behavioral and attitudinal dimensions of scientists do occur, these variations are seen as a result of nonconformity to the norm— ative system of science. Pressures to deviate from these norms arise from either the contradictory demands made on scientists by these norms or the accommodation of scientists to extra—scientific organizations. In the case of the former, for example, the norm of communality specifies that scientists must freely share under all circumstances the results of their research (Glass, 1965: 97-8; Storer, 1966: 79). Yet the striving for recognition given by the reward sys- tem of science in terms of honors, scholarly prizes, and promotion in Western based institutions often forces scientists to be secretive of their work un— til the moment of publication. These scientists may cover their research in a cloak of secrecy to prevent other scientists from "scooping" the results of their experiments and reaping the scientific rewards to which they are entitled (Merton, 1965; Reif, 1965: 142; Vanderpool, 1966: 23). ”he latt soiatior.‘ of 12331023 (9.: eateries) is $1325 in $01»? which s‘Mss JOV tists clash in b‘k'eaucratic hon t5 33332:,ojat i” U . 5°V93'Tzenta1 :75 Moe 53‘ ticiQa-tion 1.. “Essening If 33 a ppMESS 31*:1 a \- v‘ rd A. SF v' haw ‘4 1933 ICC! Po» ‘ exam . .le, 18:». "I has t'rv C . 13115 try 10 The latter case of the "conflict and accom- modation" of scientists to extra—scientific organ— izations (e.g., industrial and governmental lab— oratories) is the most documented source of varia- tions in scientific behavior. The norms of science which stress the creativity, and autonomy of scien— tists clash with the goals of product-oriented organ— izations (Glaser, 1963; Kornhauser, 1962; Marcson, 1960; Pelz and Andrews, 1967). To reduce these strains, organizational management has introduced a set of practices, such as cooptation and the as— signment of scientists to administrative positions, which have "professionalized" the developmental and bureaucratic tasks performed by scientists. From the perspective of this study, however, the accommodations scientists have made to industrial and governmental research enterprises are more important. The price scientists have had to pay for their par- ticipation in these organizations in general has been a lessening of their identification with a discipline as a profession and a strengthening of their psycholo- gical and social ties to organizational roles. They have, in the words of Glaser, become more “localite' and less "cosmopolitan" in orientation (1963: 259). For example, of all the scientific disciplines, chem- istry has the longest history of research employment by industry and labor. The result of this heritage of exposure t‘ h o: :heais ts chezists DC" '...in their chezistr'y arr? sional recosr by the: is b: oragency, 8 salaries, in; 351-“ -~ I “as Partit 11 of exposure to developmental research organizations on chemists has been a shifting of their reference groups. Rather than identifying solely with pro— fessional chemical organizations and chemists, chemists now identify with industry and government '...in their most professionalized moments" (Strauss and Rainwater, 1962: 171). No longer do chemists see their prestige as resting entirely on their contributions to the knowledge of the discipline of chemistry and the rewards for their work as profes- sional recognition. The recognition most appreciated by them is based on their contribution to the company or agency, a contribution which is rewarded by better salaries, improved working conditions and more decision- making participation in the organization. These changes of professional priorities by some chemists violate the norms of emotional neutrality and disinterestedness and have usurped the reward system of science. Adap- tation to organizational milieu is not limited to chem- istry, varying kinds of accommodations have been made by other disciplines. In summary, the literature of the sociology of science has proposed that science is structurally and normatively homogeneous. Rather than viewing homo- geneity as a problem, sociologists of science have assumed it to be universal. As a result scant at- tention is paid to the internal and external sources of heterogene tific cams: immensity 1 prophecy in t 1910;}? of sci The ass; 0? cience is ieavor. Its heterazeneoug 9333‘acterizei 0: similapii. ‘i°‘ V ~1le and th‘ in M 0 "‘35.! 0. 501 V. we nature a, 3'2‘e 01' SCier 0f scientist. hi m- Uvévodce’ the ii. 9‘ “ vi. fical e‘ 4 T‘ H slut, 12 of heterogeneity and/or homogeneity in the scien— tific community. In short, structural and normative homogeneity in science has become a self—fulfilling prophecy in the research and theorizing in the soc- iology of science. The assumed structural and normative homogeneity of science is the focal point of this research en— deavor. Its main thesis proposes that science is a heterogeneous rather than a homogeneous collectivity characterized by varyinggsets of rolesi_statuses,_and networks;gand its members maintain divergent patterns of values and lifestyles. Given the overemphasis placed on similarities in the behavior and attitudes of scien- tists and their sources in the literature of the soc- iology of science, this study will attempt to delimit the nature and sources of dissimilarities in the struc- ture of science and the behavioral and value patterns of scientists. By stressing the variant aspects of science, the areas of homogeneity or heterogeneity in science will be demarcated. In so doing, the out- come of this exploration in the area of the sociology of science will be the specification of some of the parameters of the international scientific community. In short, this dissertation will hopefully answer the following fundamental questions: is there any empirical evidence of an international scientific community as manifested in the selected population iii, if so, is eative, and ': Strecificatior. .___.___. the degree of its: a cost": azine the fol n3 LS L . S a“ Us. Vt‘vcb‘d- ‘ v R ‘1'” .m "I'E‘ *‘d, E (Viv-rt" Bud...“ ?, e-i‘ {j 0 . 3. SClentistJ T“!e - com“ 3; 13 and, if so, what are some of its structural, nor- mative, and behavioral components? §pecification of Variables. In an attempt to specify the degree of homogeneity or heterogeneity in science from a comparative perspective, this study will ex— amine the folloWing dimensions of the behavior of scientists: 1)similarities and dissimilarities in background, e.g., level of development of the home country, educational history, type of marriage, etc.; 2)variations in cross-cultural and societal ex- periences, e.g., the nature and types of extra- systemic contacts of scientists; 3)divergent or convergent patterns in the performance of scientific work roles and social roles, e.g., the social functions of scientists with regard to social change in their home country; and #)similarities and dissimilarities in the scientific and social values held by these foreign scientists, e.g., conformity to the norms of science, attitudes toward social change, etc. Two major sources of variations in these behavioral and attitudinal dimensions will form the crucial variables of this study. The first, the level of development of a scientists country of origin, is the major independent variable of this exploration. The second source, the type of work role performed by scientists, is the major intervening variable. .1? 1 b the lug the be‘navmr :ggests the". for? nee of s of lvouleige ,I In the majoril the energetics- der the isms-cj a” ,- aya“. t‘fin ~. A.V ‘1 Mr ‘I he a 14 The literature on the structure of science and the behavior of scientists in developing countries suggests that there are major differences in the per— formance of science's central task, the cultivation of knowledge, in developing and developed countries. In the majority of developing and non-Western countries, the emergence of science as an institution occurs un- der the impact of either a colonial experience, as in the case of Latin American and sub-Saharan nations, or from extensive non-colonial contact with Western civilizations, as in the case of Japan. Only in India, China, and a few of the Middle Eastern countries was science an indigeneous, early development (Needham, 19h9). However, modern science in even these instances stem predominantly from sources other than their his- torical legacy. In all of these societies, with the exception of Japan, the growth of science has not coincided with the development of their industrial and political sec- tors. Science as it exists in developed nations has been grafted into societies which do not have the com— parable supportive institutions manifest in the dev- eloped nations. As a result, the scientific enter- prise in developing areas does not function at the level it does in developed nations. Moreover, a high proportion of scientists in many developing countries have received their major professional socializatiOT- their 23‘3th internship. timing to er :repared to p 'rei to confrc szienee in ti develapin" hi with other i: mine in a rt: 2.12,“. s“. lv a“ 3 0"; or as 15 socialization to science in the developed world during their graduate training and/or their post-doctbrate internship. Expecting to begin performing or con- tinuing to enact the roles in science they have been prepared to play or have played, most scientists have had to confront the low level of development of the science in their home country. In short, science in developing nations does not interact interdependently with other institutional sectors of society, but re— mains in a marginal and oftentimes unstable position. Even though science is recognized by the leaders and representatives of these countries as important for the furtherance of modernization, science remains relatively impoverished in comparison with science in developed nations (de Solla Price, 1963: 101; Dedijer, 1962: 783; Institute of Political and Social Studies, 1967: 399-h00; united Nations, 1963a: 5 and 28; United Nations, 1963b: 17 and 31). The general malaise of science in the developing world has been characterized by a lower rate of devel- opment of science in terms of the amount of resources given to science in the forms of facilities, financial support, and integration into the general developmental planning of the nation, the number of scientists avail— able to support research activities, and viable organ- izations representing the scientific enterprise (Merton, 1965: #96). Moreover, the economic environment of which conflic suence. Sci 5‘ A .re upper uni getnerec r. t jaws-49H- a: urea...” L- v.12: is char lateratorieg 15:13:25 or i' 5'73"“ 0? a‘ it. | |A§l :' n U. ’u :" .‘JJ‘e av CM' av ivfiv..ce fro” Sigma)?“ .1 .1c n 8‘: 16 resource scarcity and limited job opportunities have generated behavioral patterns among scientists which conflict with the normative structure of science. Scientists from developing nations lack the opportunity to share the information they have gathered in their research. If they express in- dependent opinions in a hierarchical work situation which is characteristic of work relationships in laboratories and universities in some developing nations or in an authoritarian society, they pos— sibly encounter difficulties in their future scien— tific work. Such a work situation, fosters the growth of alliances based on sponsorship or nepotism rather than purely individual achievement in science (Sinha, 1970; 178-9). In these "pre—research" so— cieties, then, the demands for scientific output, both pure and applied, are high yet the inputs to science from society are modest (Dedijer, 1962: 787). Beyond their struggle to build the scientific enterprise without substantial societal support, scientists in developing nations may find that scientific research is hampered by the traditional system of values and networks of social relation- ships (Dedijer, 1962: 787; Sinha, 1970: 210). Auton- omy in the selection of problems and techniques is sometimes restricted by definitions of what is sacred azi beyoni lack of irate: traiitioral s finesse in t‘r. tne abs»: , 1 4 ‘w’ithout “3311., laCkiYif ‘in'fi “lived emc ation for me. am ' ‘ Slerlt)' an: deems 2‘0 ‘1 .39» . H 17 and beyond human understanding. In addition, the lack of integration into the social networks of traditional society where most of the social in— fluence in these societies is embedded, combined with the absence of an effective delivery system for these groups, fail to provide scientists the leverage needed to have much of an impact among these groupings. Without material and social support for their work, lacking freedom in problem selection, and with limited employment opportunities, the main consider- ation for many of these scientistsene basic economic security and the search for prestige or status. Such security, prestige, and statusare sought by obtaining work in other sectors of the society, e.g., govern— mental laboratories, civil bureaucracies, state- controlled institutions of higher education, etc. The type of positions open to scientists in these sectors are usually limited to administrative and/or teaching roles. Basic and even applied research often is subordinated to the other roles because relatively little support is given for research and there is rel- atively little utilization of results. The desire for social change by developing nations leads them to reward those work roles which have immediate sig— nificance for development and modernization of the other institutional sectors. For example, in Latin I 9‘ f'et‘ican .2111. iozirent wore: L. e' 0 ‘tfiaUlon ‘ ‘ V scientists i* 13': only resc & to perform t: if new 19,9le 18 American universities, teaching becomes the pre- dominant work role and research is shelved for the duration of the scientists professional career (Ribeiro, 1967: 3H9, 365). In short, in their search for job security and status, many of the scientists in developing nations have sacrificed not only research autonomy, but also the ability to perform tasks which are central to the production of new knowledge (Jayasuriya, n.d.: 266). Those scientists who wish to avoid these dif- ficulties in the pursuit of research activities often move across societal boundaries to societies in which research roles are rewarded. Thus, the phenomenon of the ”brain drain“ is related to the malaise of science in the developing world (Beijer, 1969: 6; committee on International Migration of Talent, 1970: #0). Given these characteristics and consequences of the systemic relationships of science to the develop- ing societies of the world we can expect, therefore, that many of the values and behavior of scientists from developing countries will differ from the soc- ial patterns of scientists from developed countries. The second major source of variations in the behavior and attitudes of scientists are the types of work roles performed by scientists. The performance by scientists of a set of work roles is only part of the total co: patterns acpr tists, as def locus of the are cociitior values, behat‘ scientist in roles are afi larger socie: fan's capa‘oii 11.1 outside c ix ideal e fitted '4 H. .1 gut.) 'i 2) an. . a)? 19 the total constellation of roles and behavioral patterns appropriate to their occupation, scien- tists, as defined by them and the institutional locus of the work situation. These work roles are conditioned by and have influence on the values, behavior and other roles performed by the scientist in his life-cycle. In addition, work roles are affected by the dynamic state of the larger society which modifies to a certain degree man's capabilities, goals, and performance within and outside of the organization of work. Six ideal types of scientific work roles can.be enacted within the variety of institutional sectors where scientists are employed: 1) Research Role: a creative role aimed at producing new knowledge, reformulating existing knowledge, solving scientific problems, and providing knowledge for solving social and technical problems; 2) Teaching-research Role: a creative of new knowledge and instructing role that is per- formed within a researbh activity which is carried on with one or more undergraduate, graduate or post-graduate students for whom this research involvement is part of their formal educational program (Henle, 1965: 14); 3) Teaching Role: a disseminating role which includes students mierrre systen; 4) Aininisti includes tration organize 5} Consult: \— | cities t 191€€ tc Power; f lecture; ”Ofefrsi K I SUCh 30: C)‘ \__l Duhsgiti' P0153 bbion ”is: eva‘ds W3 A} t Q “Trev” a“ I e, LY pg, u) 5) 6) 7) 20 includes all activities related to instructing students in formal and informal classes, both undergraduate and graduate within a university system; Administrative Role: an organization role which includes all activities related to the adminis— tration of a university, industrial or governmental organization, or research institutions and centers; Consulting: an externally oriented role that in- cludes those activities which disseminate know- ledge to the civic culture and groups holding social power; for example, consulting, delivering public lectures, etc.; Professional Role: a role whose performance includes such activities as editing of journals, membership in scientific associations and organizations, etc.; Scientific Publication Role:3 a role which includes all those activities related to the writing of professional articles, books, reports of research results, etc. 3Even though all of the other work roles may in- volve writing and/or publication, the scientific publication role has been separated from the other roles because many of the respondents considered writing and publication of professional articles and books as the only work role they have or are currently performing. From the I H ‘ ‘ r 5..ch dare 0o seiner, teacr central to 5' other work r — ' — — — rational sec scientists p to vary ing (1. researcher ! tinistratc . can POSit a 21 From the perspective of the traditional normative structure of science on a world—wide scale, the roles of re- search,teaching-research,professional,and publication are central to science and are normatively valued. The other work roles are central to the respective insti- tutional sector and peripheral to science. Moreover, scientists perform one or more of these work roles to varying degree, i.e., some scientists are only researchers, others are researchers, teachers, and administrators at the same time. Accordingly, one can posit a series of hypotheses relating the degree to which these types of work roles are performed by scientists and the behavior they enact, functions they fulfill, and values they share. In addition, these types of work roles are important because of the variations which they pro- duce on such phenomena as interaction patterns in the world community of science, the international movements of scientists, and the location of scien- tists in the process of modernization in their re- spective home countries. With reference to the world community of scientists, formal and informal inter- action patterns are established and maintained by scientists according to the type of work roles they perform. Researchers tend to interact with other researchers and support organizations which act as a focal point and context of scientific communication of research i hreover, as stiz'ili to t1 ietal bonni'i "train drain providing a Work roles ( 131). Final tribute to t in ; “Vesvizati: tific work 1' the Elobai '1' . , a; agientls a 22 of research information (Kaplan and Storer, 1968). Moreover, as was stated previously, one of the main stimuli to the movement of scientists across soc- ietal boundaries and the recurrent phenomenon of the “brain drain“ is the failure of many societies in providing a fruitful context in which to pursue these work roles (Porter;,1968: 7; de Solla Price, 1963: 101). Finally, the manner in which scientists con- tribute to the development of their specific society is structured along work role line. Hence, this einvestigation of the performance of types of scien- ‘tific work roles provides a useful base for viewing 1the global dimensions of the behavior and orientations (:f scientists. Le JEDISsociative Experiences. The model of the development of |'third cultural" networks and groupings as formulated by Useem, Useem and Donoghue (1963) and the model of ltnodernization constructed by waisanen (1969) provide a useful scheme for explicating the effect of level of development of a scientist's home country and the The term “dissociative experience" has been used Gaxtensively in the field of psychiatry. I am not ‘Iasing it in a psychological manner here, but I am 313efering to the sociological and social psychological aspects of dissociative experiences. Thusly, on the Tluext page I will define dissociative experiences as Inovement from one social system to another or contact "Vith the members of an exogeneous social system by ‘51 social actor. work 13.3165 p) 1%?) Either $93.5 lhich rite” of d mired in r e to each othe revelers-“rent crar3“‘°”ize v Vv‘ r. a- «aelofig. U do! {4193 Hm atone) Sister ‘p u CC V 1 .4) e ‘mfiou or? .y cav- 23 work roles performed by him on the dependent variables to be discussed below. Both of these models emphasize the importance of dissociative experiences (Waisanen, 1969) either in the formation of ”...cultural pat- terns which are created, learned, and shared by the members of different societies who are personally in- volved in relating their societies, or sections thereof, to each other“ (Useem and Useem, 1967: 130) or in the development of modernization at an individual level «characterized by innovativeness, knowledgeability and «evaluation, future time orientation, commitment to :Eflanning, and a belief in the calculability of events éand their control through science and technology (Waisanen and Kumata, 1969: 2). Dissociative experiences can be specified as the actual movement of a social actor from one social sys- tem to another social system or series of social systems and as exposure to the ideas, technology, and social norms and cultural values of a "foreign" social system or systems through contact with actors from other soc- ieties and cultures, with the media of other systems, Or with the material culture of adjacent or distant Jnations. Hence, dissociative experiences are the ave— 3'lues through which the elements of two or more social Systems become articulated (Loomis and Loomis, 1965: ‘1.6). Through physical and/or psychic mobility not (Inly can social actors gain access to exogeneous so- c331.611 and cultural milieu, they can also act as representati In thei rial systems tween the so they share i tractions s end statuses is in this xi Sharing thy iereloped, 031) 033.3051 EhZv'Sical ani 3r inputs ar ca: be a re( Whether emeriER-n e S c131 System 21+ representatives of their own social system. In their encounters with members of other so- cial systems, these individuals act as mediators be- tween the social systems. As “men—in-the-middle," they share in their binational and multinational in— teractions some of the values and life—styles, roles and statuses, and technology of their society. It is in this process of extra-systemic contact and sharing that a set of third cultural social structures developed. In short, dissociative experiences are not only composed of inputs from one system to another. Physical and psychic mobility also involve an exchange Of inputs and outputs. Dissociative experiences, then, can ‘be a reciprocal process of systemic interaction. Whether a social actor is exposed to dissociative experiences or not depends upon the nature of the so- eial system he is in and the degree to which the actor is embedded in that system, that is, circumscribed by the system (Waisanen, 1969: 3-7). The social norma— tiVe structure of the social system can either place constraints on the degree to which social actors can pa-:f‘1::icipate in systems exogeneous to itself or it may act as a mobilizing agent pushing the actor into con- tact with outside systems. Embeddedness in the social 8t’rs‘bem can be defined as the relationship of an actor to the social system (Waisanen, 1969: 7). If the actors participation in the social system encapsulates :he actor to eczess to 0t} other hand, i flexicle and acial syste ani actual p reciaale ,t'n the systemic ex;erienee a 25 the actor totally in that social system, the actor's access to other systems will be curtailed. If, on the other hand, his relationship to the social system is flexible and not fixed, his ability to encounter other social systems in interpersonal relations, media use, and actual physical movement is enhanced. The first variable, the nature of the social system, delimits the systemic boundaries of possible inter-systemic experience and the second variable defines the role boundaries of possible systemic linkage. The former can be called systemic circumscription and the latter, role circumscription. In terms of the problem at hand, dissociative ex- Periences are the first dependent variable of the study and an intervening variable. Using level of development or a scientist's home country as a index of systemic circumscription, and the type of scientific work role Performed by the scientist as an index of role circum- scription, the degree‘ of a scientist's exposure to so- cial systems outside of his home country will vary with these variables. Following the literature in the sociology of science as discussed in the prior section, scientists who perform research, teaching-research, and professional and publi— cation roles will be more physically and psychically In"mile than scientists who perform teaching, ad- ministrative, and public roles. In other words, the former a latter. In iepends upor. co‘otm', sys following for Propositic fiODOSit :‘ D —- ’ — — — — 26 the former are less role circumscribed than the latter. In addition, performance of these roles depends upon the level of development of the home country, systemic circumscription. Therefore, the following propositions are proposed: Proposition 1: The greater the systemic ' circumscription as evidenced by the level of development of the home country, then, the greater the role circum- scription, as exemplified in performance of teaching, adm- inistrative, and public roles. Ifiwoposition 2: The greater the role circum- scription, the lesser the ex- posure to dissociative ex- periences. Systemic circumscription, as evidenced in level 0f development of home country, will be measured by Harbison's and Myers' (1964) classification of nations by levels of human resource development. Using a comPosite index of human resource development, Harbison and.PWer ranked 75 nations (1964: 26-3h). The index is composed of nine measures: 1)number of teachers P9P 10,000 population; 2)angineers and scientists p31” 10,000 population; 3)physicians and dentists per 10.000 population; Mpuplls enrolled at first level (PPimary) education as percentage of the estimated pOpulation aged 5 to 11+ inclusive; 5)adjusted school em'OIlment for first and second level (secondary) education combined; 6)pupils enrolled at second level edJJ.C:ation as a percentage of the estimated population ‘889d.15 to 19 inclusive, adjusted for length of schooling; 7Eertolleent 5 . . a oercenage fazultles 1r. stoients eh.“ i 5 ins ans, 5 Hart 3: developzent nations. 1; consider as element. j .3 for Elise: as toe IVerl 112g bECWeer it'lelopedl Y Max:331 aa: a voctpiec v “o H" 'ifi h ovngr. 77' «ne 'ka ukltei 27 7)enrollment in third level (higher) education as a percentage of age group 20 to 24; 8)percentage of students enrolled in scientific and technical faculties in a recent year; and 9)percentage of students enrolled in faculties of humanities, fine arts, and law in the same year. Harbison and Myers formulated four levels of development upon the basis of the scores of the nations. Level 1 is composed of those nations they consider as I‘underdeveloped" in human resource dev— elopment. The range of scores in level 1 is from .3 for Niger to 7.55 for Sudan with countries such ‘88 the Ivory Coast, Congo, Haiti and Senegal fal- ling between these scores. Level 2, “partially developed” nations range from Guatemala and Indonesia at 10.7 to Iraq at 31.2. Between these extremes lie such nations as Mainland China, Turkey, Paraquay, and Pakistan. In level 3, "semi-advanced" cOuntries, nations such as Czechoslovakia, Poland, India, South Korea, Cuba, etc. fall between the 8cores of 33.0 for Mexico to 73.8 for Norway. Six- teen nations are classified in level 4, "developed”, with Denmark with a score of 77.1 at one extreme "itfll the United States with a score of 261.3 at the °ther. The country whose score is closest to the United States is New Zealand at 147.3. (* Since level 3 is r. level it and to thi net: (see Interns :ortries ir. as developir. siierei dove the sixteen as aivanced as being fr: scriptign). nations m; nations (hi: een develor «111 3d» H8“ filiapk. 8‘ been +» ‘0 ‘ Iva?“ Ing’ k“ eseal‘ch vv‘Vitieq hOEe 28 Since the range of scores between level 1 and level 3 is not as great as the range of scores in level 4 and since most of the scientists who come to this nation on visits are from levels 3 and u (see International Institute of Education, 1968), countries in level 1, 2, and 3 will be classified as developing and countries in level it will be con- sidered developed. Hence, foreign scientists from the sixteen nations Harbison and Myers classified as advanced will be considered in this investigation as being from developed nations (low systemic circum- scription). Foreign scientists from the 59 other nations will be considered as coming from developing nations (high systemic circumscription). The six- teen developed nations, in descending rank order, are United States, New Zealand, Australia, Netherlands, Belgium, United Kingdom, Japan, France, Canada, U.S.S.R., F inland, West Germany, Israel, Argentina, Sweden and Denmark. Since no scientists from the United States are part of the population of this study, there are only fifteen nations from the developed'levelo Role circumscription will be measured by the time and effort scientists estimate they spend in team-1mg, basic research, applied research, teaching- reSearch, administration, consulting, organizational activities, and writing and publication while in their home country (see questions 17 on the Questionnaire an 28 and 2 peniix A). their time a | l: eioinistr circus-arise and effort 1 research, or (profession: are consiier Disses". physical an: “111 be sea-3|I 1. Couotr: SCience n81re e I 20 Trips ‘ (quest: scheduj 30 CrOSS--i 10’ qr‘il rs.Vchic not: 03 these its 1" Intera< Co‘mtr' 17: 111‘ 2' Co‘mtm sociatj 31 inte T 2“} ' ctever‘actlh? leek “vet‘s-hips 0" . '36? man e \ 3‘ ,., .. ‘ szcnlc ? 29 and 28 and 29 on the Interview Schedule in Ap— pendix A). Those scientists who spend most of their time and effort teaching, consulting, and in administration will be considered as high role circumscribed. Scientists who spend their time and effort in basic and applied research, teaching— research, organizational activities in science (professional role), and writing and publication are considered as low role circumscribed. Dissociative experiences will be divided into Physical and psychic mobility. Physical mobility Will be measured by the following items: ‘1. Country or countries where socialization to science has been received (question 9, question- naire and interview schedule); 2. Trips to developed and developing countries (question 11, questionnaire; 14, interview schedule); 3. Cross-societal experience in work (question 10, questionnaire and interview schedule). Psychic mobility, on the other hand, will be measured by these items: 11. Interaction with foreigners in the home country (question 12, uestionnaire and 17, interview schedule ; 2. Country of memberships in scientific as- sociations (Question 46, questionnaire, 81 interview schedule). Inter-acting with foreigners in the home country and memberships in scientific associations in countries other than the home country are regarded as indices or Psychic mobility because ideas, values, and of an exogef AS Stat are or“161211 social strué attituies "E- this study 1 dissociative as a depenié variable' filth 1"" social strucl .wsltiOn is tropositi': ial 3‘ social and ‘ they have h‘ ez‘2e“s of C Se #u .res. "” J19 of t‘ 30 behavioral patterns of other social systems can be obtained through such contact with the members of an exogeneous social system. As stated previously, dissociative experiences are crucial to the development of third cultural social structures networks and the formation of attitudes which reflect individual modernity. Since this study is concerned with both of these issues, dissociative experiences will be considered not only as a.d00fi0000fl3 ho onkg oouhmvdrnmwaxufl Okrdwbflwfiocmnufl: 1H0 mumvnnknbw ..l~x~d>.eufl~> Gnu fwohwfiphoanxvfl .H 1U>svl~ LC who .H 3 Wish dfhnu .04: fiv..r\-.\m~ :eCLeafi u \. eqnu I}? ANNNuzv Ro.ooH 0.3a @.N 3.00 ao.mm macapmao somma ones» Inmaom swaonom a“ mmanmaoneoz No Ammmuzv Ammmuzv Ammmuzv Ammmuzv Ammmuzv mo.00H Ro.oofi mo.ooa fio.cofi Ro.oofi 'III IIIII II'I. Ill' m o H e.m m.n o.oH o.m o.sfi m.ms m.mu w.ws m.mw :.ws mm.mm &H.sfi mm.ms mm.om ms.mm hpussoo oaom mofiupssoo moappssoo ooqodaoaxm cocoapoaxm on» :« wsfimoaosmn uonoao>oa xpoz Hmsoflpmozdm macswfioaom on wedge on agape smfioaom .Q.:m cwdopom spa: scapompcpsH “moaoaamaxm o>apmd00mman he came mosoanodxm cpapmHOOmmHQ mo momma wsaznm> on onshoawm Ho>oq mo scapunanpman omouscopom “a canoe "Hosea mQHSmamneoe 02\.Q.£m oz omsoamomrsoz 30g swam "ooaoaaoaxm o>apmHoOmmaa no Hc>oq matrix of physical ( systemic a iissociati correlate: 63 matrix of the relationships among the psychic and physical mobility variables and their connection to systemic and role circumscription, discloses that the dissociative experience variables are for the most part correlated with one another in a positive direction. Obtaining a doctorate in a foreign country is positively correlated with Foreign Work Experience, Trips to Devel- oping Country, Interaction with Foreigners, and Member- ships in Foreign Scientific Associations, and weakly associated with Trips to Developed Countries. Making trips to developed countries is positively associated with Trips to Developing Countries, Interaction with Foreigners, and Memberships Country. Similarly, making trips to developing countries is positively correlated with interaction in the home country with individuals from other nations, and belonging to scientific as- sociations in other nations. Only in two sets of cor- relations do negative associations appear. Working in a foreign country is negatively correlated with belonging to foreign scientific as- sociations, negatively associated in weak manner with Trips to Developing Countries, and unrelated to Trips to Developed Countries and Interaction with Foreigners. In other words, those scientists who have worked abroad before the present period tend to belong only to their home country scientific associations and have made no prior trips to developed countries. One 7 lie in th quite you reason gi' by the re s. iately tation gr; by the co or its so work EXpe abroad , t _| a: 64 One possible explanation for this occurrence may lie in the fact that the scientists in the sample are quite young, mean age of 32, and the most frequent reason given for the cross-national work experience by the respondents is that they were employed im— mediately after obtaining the doctorate by the insti- tution granting the degree. The latter is supported by the correlation of .618 between receiving a Ph.D. or its equivalent in a foreign country and foreign work experience. After working for a year or two abroad, they return home for a brief period and are again physically mobile, as evidencedby their current stay in the united States. During their short stay at home and given their very brief professional career; they have had neither . the time nor experience to develop those networks which would stimulate both physical and psychic mobility. One would expect, however, that upon their return to the home country after this trip to the united States and after they have established a professional career in their nation, their exper- iences abroad would stimulate their capacity to be exposed to other types of dissociative experiences. The second set of variables which reveal a neg- ative relationship with one another are the two psychic mobility indices. Interacting with foreigners in the home country coincides with belonging to scientific associations only in the home country. Again this l lllllllll|l|llllllllulllall . |.lll| II I .. .. virithfiféa {tractLtan ca thLL. Cu tCrLL. thwECk .Qtzl CkaLOk IESDLfiD OHOQ OfiEGQD>W nCOCDdLOQKE ObdulAOOnndQ “In! COflOQfiLODESEflU s. CH0: 1:! 035000on .HO undLOl—l HIECfiO'H-YHLCU ~mw DHDOK. .sowpdfiAomsfikoo each so coapwozw on» on momsoamohuco: mo sundown .wma ma soapmahomESoaHo each you 2 one .coapnwhomsaonwo mach mo mcoapmaohhoo can you uaooxo .NNN ma cane» wasp you 2 one * mmm.u In--- mmm. mmm. mam. no“. mom.u wmo.u ohm. om". om n. RH? moo. mmo. mcoapuwoomm< oamapcofiom naocwaohom :32“; fl as“: madcmhanoz :cauouthCH * moosouuoawm c>apqwoommaa was sowvmahon2§ondo New. IIIII :Na.u duo. cum. Now. can. odfi. Hms. mmo.u moaup Icaco mad noahpsdoo Iaoam>oa podoacboa op amass on adage mac. as: mma. oOCOflhon Ixm xhoz cwaouom Nmo.c mom. oocoahoawm Hucoaveospm dam a398,.“ -5230 30m wwm. coavaahon oaom 8. 3239mm mo fist: Héfipflofioo .m tapas uIIuI mcoapsaoommd owmfiucowom :wdchom CH emanmhanoz IuIII myocwacnom new: cowpocaoch niece moahpndoo meadoa Imboa op mafiae IeIII mcfihpcfioo pcmoa uc>on on nudge ennui ooccwhomxm 0733 :waohom IIIII cocoahomxm Hucoapuospm ohm-Sm fiWflgoh IIIuI coavdfihom 1.50%an OHOm IIIII soapmahom nadohfio oesopmmm cowuddhon assuage edSopuhm eopirica' of the r- negative pretatic systemic Scientif developi sociati: 66 empirical relationship is opposite of the expectations of the researcher. A tentative explanation of this negative relationship can be offered through an inter— pretation of the very strong association of .903 between systemic circumscription and Memberships in Foreign Scientific Associations in Table 8. Scientists from developing countries often belong to scientific as- sociations outside of their home country. This rela_ tionship implies that,given a low level of development of the scientific and educational institutions of the home country, scientists must go out of their home country to participate in scientific associations which may or may not exist in wemrown country. The scientist from a developed nation does not need to do this. He can remain in the home country to receive and contribute information about current developments in the field. Moreover, he does not need to establish interaction patterns with foreigners. The scientist from a devel- oped nation can rely upon his own countrymen for the information he desires and for colleagues. In short, scientists from developing nations must go out of their home country to keep up with current developments in the field and to make themselves known to and come to know the larger scientific community. The scientists from developed nations, on the other hand, can rely on their home country's scientific and educational assets. 67 Systemic and Role Circumscription vs. Dissociative Ex- perience. Table 8 also presents the correlations among systemic and role circumscription and the dissociative experience variables. In examining the results presented in the table, one finds that systemic circumscription and role circumscription is related to the dissociative experience variables in a reverse direction than ex- pected, i.e., the higher the systemic and role circum- scription, the higher the level of exposure to dis- sociative experiences. In addition, the correlations between systemic circumscription and the dissociative experience indices are greater than the relationship of role circumscription to dissociative experiences. Systemic circumscription is related to Foreign Ph.D. Educational Experience, Foreign Work Experience, Trips to Developing Countries, and Memberships in Foreign Scientific Associations and unrelated to Trips to Devel- oped Countries and Interaction with Foreigners. Role circumscription is related to Trips to Developing Coun- tries and Memberships in Foreign Scientific Associations, slightly associated with Trips to Developed Countries and Interaction with Foreigners, and unrelated to Foreign Ph.D. Educational Experience and Foreign Work Experience. In other words scientists from developing nations have cross-societal educational and work experiences, make trips to other developing nations and belong to asso- ciations in other countries (a point which has been 68 discussed previously). Moreover, those scientists who teach, administrate, or consult in their home country have made trips to developing countries and belong to scienfitic organizations in other countries. They also possibly make trips to developed countries and do not necessarily interact with foreigners in their home countries. No relationship exists between their performance of these work roles and cores—societal educational and work experiences. Given these findings, Proposition 2: The greater the role circumscription, the lesser the exposure to dissociative experience cannot be accepted nor re- jected and in fact leans towards rejection. However, the association between systemic circumscription and role circumscription may be confounding these results. In order to show if in fact this is the case, Table 9 was constructed in which level of systemic circumscrip— tion is controlled. Controlling for a high level of systemic circum- scription, the relationship between role circumscrip- tion and exposure to dissociative experiences is strength- ened. Scientists from developing nations who perform teaching, administrative or consulting roles are more likely to be physically and psychically mobile than those who perform research, teaching—research, organ- ization and publication roles. This result is the reverse of the relationship implied in proposition 2. H v r» at «v.0«L...E:CO KCTLO~C>Qly she: cofiunaLcc520Lfio antum>m COfiquLOEESCLfiU OfiEOQTRW LO-“ .QCAHqOLUCOO .. 002.0 7.512734 C> «.useuCOQEWQ uhCHDfiAL— FLUIEW—CLIF.O [ENIGMA :nficznfibVTfi anhAU «\U ~..NN-ELLPVU u 0 OHS 5.5 BOA wmm. msoHpmaoOmma canapsoaom smaoaoa ca anaempmneoz one. mSOapmHUOmw< camapsmfiom smfionom :H maanmponeoz mmd. mpmsmaosom Spa: seapomsmpaH as . mamsmaopom £ua3 scapomsopsH mm .mcfipflpz dam :oHpmoaHpsa camapsmfiom cam .mmapfi>apom Hmsofimmomoam .ncpmommpiwsasomop .30LMmmmu madmaosa aofipaaaomssopflo mace .pHSmsoo so .mpmgpmasaeem .sommp on: emcee mmuSHosfi scapaasomesoaao macs sweat :mm.u mum. NHH. mam.l mmampnsoo moappssoo mosmdamaxm mosoapomxm weamoac>om dmaoao>om xnoz HonoHpmozcm cs mange on waste essence .q.:m swfiopom Asmsuzv Amoflppqsoo concao>mmv sou mmH. «ms. was.u mmfi. mofiapssoo moaapssoo mosmapomxm mommapoaxm mammofio>oa emaoam>oa xpoz HmaofimeSUm as waste op waste essence .a.:m Qwfioaom Afiofiuzv mmoaoapoaxm o>apmH60mmHQ Amoappnsoo mammoao>omv swam cofiumapOmezopao caempmam sowpaHLOmesopao oweopmmm pom weaaaospsoo "moscapcaxm m>apmH00mmHo cam sodpmapomesopdo oHom gmozpom msofipmaoshoo no canoe escapafipom easonao maom escapmaaom Iezoafio oaom Only wit work Ex: proposit of role societai Tr ; and exp: SUSIE? . I. tists 1‘: 312112131 to deve belong Opposit 70 Only with reference to work experience abroad, Foreign Work Experience, is the association supportive of the proposition, i.e., those scientists with a high level of role circumscription are less likely to have cross- societal work experiences. The relationship between role circumscription and exposure to dissociative experience is again strengthened for low systemic circumscription. Scien- tists from developed nations who perform teaching, administrative or consulting roles usually make trips to developed countries, interact with foreigners, and belong to scientific associations in other nations. In opposition to the finding that scientists from devel- oping nations who have a high level of role circum-- scription have less cross-societal work experiences, Table 9 shows that Foreign Work Experience is slightly associated with role circumscription for those scien- tists from developed nations. Moreover the correlations between role circumscription and Foreign Ph.D. Educational Experience and Trips to Developing Countries are in the reverse direction for low systemically circumscribed scien— tists. Scientists who are from developed nations and who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles are less likely to go abroad for their doctorate or its equi- valent and are less likely to make trips to developing coun- tries. Both of these findings are supportive of prop- osition 2. However, caution is needed in interpreting the corr and role of syste scienti: to make tists f: from de' may be 1 Table 8 t0 impl role c1 fDr‘mula of role pOS‘fl-J‘e 71 the correlation between Trips to Developing Countries and role circumscription because of the relationship of systemic circumscription to this variable. Since scientists from a developing nation are more likely to make trips to other developing nations than scien— tists from developed nations (only thirteen scientists from developed nations made such trips), the correlation may be meaningless for scientists from developed nations. These results along with the finding presented in Table 8 indicate that proposition 2 needs to be recast to imply a reverse direction of the relationship between role circumscription and dissociative experiences. Re- formulated proposition 2 states, the higher the level of role circumscription, the higher the level of ex— posure to dissociative experiences. This reformulation of proposition 2 when viewed in conjunction with proposition 1 implies that scien— tists from developing countries who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles are more likely to be exposed to dissociative experiences than their col- leagues who perform research, teaching-research, pro- fessional, and publication roles. Moreover, in a sim- ilar fashion, scientists from developed nations with a high level of role circumscription are more physically and psychically mobile than their colleagues who perform roles which have been categorized as low level role .‘C I V n.” .‘I' . - C. lI‘CUTS Tl? 72 circumscription. The confirmation of proposition 1 supports the argument made in the theoretical section of the study that scientists from developing countries perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles, in part, because of the lack of available resources to support the other scientific work roles. Given the low level of development of educational and scientific institutions, many of these scientists must go out of their countries to make themselves visible to their field, to get in- formation on current developments, and to establish colleagial relationships with other scientists. It may also be possible that the performance of teaching, administrative, or consulting roles opens avenues through which experiences in other societies and with members of these societies can be gained. Since these roles are organizationally embedded, the scientist can rely upon the functional position he occupies in the organization to give him access to cross-societal experiences or he can use his position to obtain such access. Such a scientist is more “visible" than the research scientist whose visibility is determined by his periodic publications in his home country journals and the recognition accorded to them. For example, when visitors from other nations arrive at a research or ed— ucational institution for a tour of its facilities, they will most likely meet in extensive contact administrators may rel obtain ihether .‘V‘ be pros 73 or teachers rather than the researchers in the lab— oratory. Or the administrator or consultant may make trips to other nations to observe other organ- izations perform their tasks, whereas the researcher may rely more heavily on written communication to obtain information about work being done elsewhere. Whether this explanation is tenable or not can only be proved in future research in which a stress is placed on the visibility of the performance of varying types of scientific work roles. In summary, these findings indicate that there are differences in the behavioral patterns of scien- tists. Some of them have had more exposure to dis- sociative experiences than others. The differences in exposure to varying types of experiences in other societies and with their members is related to the level of development of the respondents home countries (systemic circumscription) and the type of work roles they have performed in their countries of origin (role circumscription). Moreover, scientists from developing coun- tries perform :roles in the work situation which are different from those played by scientists from devel- oped nations. These results support the ideas that level of development of the scientists' home country and the types of work roles performed by them are important sources of variations in the behavior of scientists in the international scientific community. ucation Th in that cent of Years 0 thirtyi 74 Additional Characteristics and Backgrounds of the Respondents. In this chapter, we have already analyzed portions of the background and characteristics of the visiting foreign scientists in this study. It has been shown that they have not been, as a whole, physically and psychically mobile and that they usually perform in their home countries roles which have been defined as low role circumscribing. This section explores additional attributes of the respondents with reference to their marital and familial backgrounds, their ed- ucational experiences, and their employment histories. The first major characteristic of the respondents in the study is their age. As Table 10 shows, 40.1 per cent of the scientists are between twenty-six and thirty years of age and 29.8 per cent between thirty-one and thirty-five. Only 6.7 per cent are over forty. With Table 10: Age Distribution of the Respondents Age Grouping of the Respondents Per Cent Under 25 5.8% 26-30 40.1 31-35 29.8 36-40 16.7 41 and Over 6.7 Non-Response .9 Total 100.0% (N=222) Mean age = 32.0 Y—_ a mean a ; genera .. ferenee nation: 75 a mean age of 32, then, we are dealing with a younger generation of scientists. There are no significant dif- ferences between scientists from developed and developing nations in terms of age. Our study has tapped a generational segment in the international scientific community which is post-war and, in many cases, post-independence in terms of the scientists from former colonial nations. In addition, given their youth, these scientists have been involved in the scientific and professional activities of their fields for only a short period. Most of the respondents received their doctorate after the 1955, as Table 11 indicates. Only 3.7 per cent of the respondents obtained their Ph.D. before 1956. Since this study was conducted in 1969, the maximum number of Table 11: Percentage Distribution of Years in Which the Doctorate was Received by the Respondents Years in which Doctorate was Received by the Respondents: Per Cent Prior 1950 1.4% 1950-1955 2.3 1956-1960 7.2 1961-1965 24.3 1966 and Over 40.5 Doctorate in Progress 5.4 No Doctorate 2-3 Non-Response 16.6 Total 100.0% (N=222) years n profess status in this are pro 76 years most respondents could have been active in a professional life after their emergence from a student status is twelve years. As a result, the respondents in this study are not only young in age, but also they 11 are professionally young. The visiting foreign scholars usually complete their highest degree within a relatively short period of time after receiving their bachelor's degree. Four- tenths obtained their highest degree one to five years after they obtained their first degree, as Table 12 dis- closes. Only 10.0 per cent take longer than ten years to finish the educational process. These results in- dicate that the majority of the respondents do not ex- perience any breaks between their undergraduate and graduate education. Those scientists who do take longer to complete their highest degree generally cite such factors as military service and lack of financial assets as the major reasons why they did not go directly on into graduate school. Others pointed to another reason, they were unsure of their career goals and vocations after they received their bachelor's degree and, as a consequence, delayed going into graduate school until they had arrived at what they considered to be an ap— propriate decision. For these scientists, the gap between graduate and undergraduate education is a trial and error period in which they are employed in a variety of tasks in and outside of the scientific 11 The respondents are also almost all males. Only twelve of the scientists are females. .4 I r.‘ *3 77 community exploring alternative career routes. Table 12: Percentage Distribution of Length of Years Between Bachelor's Degree and the Highest Degree Received by the Respondents Length of Years Between Bachelor's Degree and Highest Degree Received by the Respondents: Per Cent 1-5 Years 43.7% 6—10 Years 34.2 11-15 Years 7.? 16Years and Over 2.3 Non-Response 12.1 Total: 100.0% (N=222) In the section on dissociative experiences, foreign educational experience at the doctoral level was examined. No reference was made, however, to the level of development of the countries in which the scientists received their Ph.D. Here we will explore not only the level of development of the country in which the doctorate was obtained, but also of those countries in which the bachelor's and master's degrees were received. Table 13 examines the relationship of the re- spondents' systemic circumscription, i.e., the level of development of the country of origin, and the level of development of the country in which they received their undergraduate degree. Almost all of the scien- tists receive their undergraduate training in their 8'53 .1 WS haVe 78 home countries. In addition, only eleven of the respondents took their bachelor's degree in coun- tries other than their home countries. In thier undergraduate training the scientists in this study do not cross national boundaries. Table 13: Systemic Circumscription and the Level of Development of the Country in Which the Bachelor's Degree was Received by the Respondents Level of Development of the Country in Which the Bachelor's Systemic Circumscription Degree was Received by the Respondents High (Developing) Low (Developed) Developing 82.2% 4.1% Developed 5.9 78.5 Non-Response 11.9 17.4 Total: 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) At the master's level, the scientists again usually receive their degree in countries having a similar level of development as their home country. But as Table 14 shows, none of the scientists from developed countries have completed their master's in developing countries. 23.1 per cent of the respondents from developing countries, on the ohter hand, went to developed countries for their initial graduate training. In probes on the question of the country in which the master's degree was received, thirteen of the scientists from developed countries obtained their M.A.'s or M.S.'s in developed countries 79 different from their home countries. None of the scientists from developing nations received their master's degrees in foreign developing nations. These probes reveal that scientists from developing countries are less likely to go to nations with a similar level of development for their initial graduate training than scientists from developed nations. Scientists from developing nations, however, are more cross-development mobile than scientists from developed nations. At the doctoral level, we find in Table 15 that scientists from developing nations go to developed nations for their Ph.D.‘s, but none of the scientists from developed countries receive their doctorate in developing nations. The probes to the question revealed that only one scientist from a developing country went to another developing country for his doctorate. In contradistinction, thirty scientists from developed nations travelled to other developed countries for their Ph.D. Therefore, if scientists from developing countries go abroad for their graduate training at both the master's and doctoral level, they usually go to a developed country and not to a developing country. Scientists from devel- oped countries avoid any of the developing countries for a graduate educational experience. If they go abroad, they will go to a country having a similar level of development as their own. At the bachelor's level, both the scientists from developed and developing nations do not generally have foreign educational experience. T4? Lew: 2? a. 900th b3" ti? 80 Table 14: Systemic Circumscription and the Level of Development of the Country in Which the Master's Degree was Received by the Respondents Level of Development of Country in Which the Master's Degree Systemic Circumscription was Received by the Respondnets High (Developing) Low (Developed) Developing 49.0% - - - Developed 23.1 42.1 No Degree 4.8 17.1 Non—Response 23.1 40.8 Total: 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) Table 15: Systemic Circumscription and the Level of Development of the Country in Which the Doctorate was Received by the Respondents Level of Development of Country in Which the Doctorate was Received by the Respondents High (Developing) Low (Developed) Systemic Circumscription Developing 39.3% _-_- Developed 33.6 91.6 No Degree 1.9 .8 Non-Response 25.2 7.6 Total: 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) 111 youthf‘ the re "-— with n' are 31: 71.2 p per cc and 6.| quarts 81 Besides being young in age and professionally youthful with varying educational experiences abroad, the respondents in the study generally are married with no children. Only 27.5 per cent of the respondents are single and .9 per cent are divorced. Of those 71.2 per cent of the respondents who are married, 15.3 per cent have one to two children in their families and 6.8 per cent have three or more children. Three- quarters of the respondents, then, are childless. In the interview schedule, the respondents were asked if their spouses and children accompanied them on their current trip to the United States. Almost all of them replied in the affirmative. When the scientists were asked why their families made the journey with them, the most frequent response was that they wanted their families to have an experience in a foreign country and to see the splendor of, as one Indian biologist related, “super rich America.“ Only a few scientists' spouses travelled with them for pro- fessional purposes. In general, then, for the families of these scientists the current stay in the United States is a vacation, visit, or sightseeing tour. Given this purpose, many of the scientists mentioned, in passing comments, that they felt obligated to their spouse and children to show them the varying aspects of American culture and life. As one Australian math- ematical biologist said, "on weekends, my wife and two t..'-;)!-u1-—a— sons 1 the s; and 0f seeker also Spous 82 sons take off in our microbus and tour the city and the suburbs. Often we take trips to Indiana, Wisconsin, and other neighboring states. When I have to work the weekends, I feel I have disappointed my family. All week their cooped up in the apartment. This is the only time they get to see the country and its people. We're planing to delay our return home for several weeks so that we can make up for lost sightseeing." This scientist and several others discussed the tension of their conflict with work and family. But the general pattern seems to be an absence of conflict between the world of work and family in the current stay in the United States. On the interview schedule, the scientists were also asked to give background information on their spouses, and parents (questions 11, 12, 13, interview schedule). The first item of information deals with the citizenship of these individuals. Only two scien- tists from developed countries married spouses from foreign developed nations and none of the scientists from developing countries married a spouse from a foreign country. With regard to their parents, only five of the eighty-two respondents to this interview schedule have mothers whose citizenship differs from theirs. Of these five scientists, four are from devel- oped countries and one from a developing country. Similarly, only four of the respondents' fathers are backghl point $90324 respoy feren; 83 from countries different from theirs. Three of the scientists are from developed countries and one from a developing country. In general, the respondents in the study, do not have cross-cultural marriages or paren— tage, although 11 per cent have cross-cultural marriages. The second type of data on spouse and familial background probes into their birthplace. Here the point of interest is whether or not their parents or spouses were cross-societally mobile. Only six of the respondents' spouses had citizenships which were dif- ferent then their birthplace. Seven of the respondents' mothers and nine of the respondents' fathers were born in countries different from the nations in which they currently hold citizenships. There are no differences between the scientists from developed and developing nations on the mobility of their parents or spouses. In general, then, they do not marry cross—culturally and their family backgrounds are usually limited to one society, their home country. The third item of information on the familial and spouse characteristics is their occupational back- grounds. Table 16 presents the occupations of the fathers, mothers, and spouses of the respondents. Since an important aspect of the occupational back- grounds of these individuals is whether or not they are in scientific fields identical or different from the respondents or in other academic, non-scientific fields, the occupational categories have been divided I- "'1 a! into twc fields, cupatioz As have fa: science 0f the 1 into thi e.g,, i: 0? the 1 are in 5 majorit; Spouses In the majc “SinesE gOVepnmE of their 84 into two categories: a)science and other academic fields, and b)non-scientific and non-academic oc- cupations. As Table 16, shows, 7.3 per cent of the respondents have fathers and spouses in the same field of science as they are in. Only 1.2 per cent have mothers in a field identical to their own. In terms of fields in science different from the respondents, 9.8 per cent of the fathers, and 13.4 per cent of their spouses fall into this category. In non-scientific academic positons, e.g., in the humanities, history, etc., only 1.2 per cent of the fathers and mothers and 6.1 per cent of the spouses are in such fields. These results indicate that the majority of scientists in this study have parents or spouses who are not in scientific and academic occupations. In the non-scientific and non-academic category, the major occupations of the scientists' fathers are business, government service, e.g., federal officer, government bureaucrat, etc., and labor (worker). Most of their mothers and spouses are unemployed. Of those who are employed, the major categories of employment are laborer, and school teacher for mothers and school teacher for spouses. Using the data on fathers' occupation as a crude index of class background of the respondents, one can conclude the majority of respondents come from a white-collar and a professional class. There were no discernible differences between scientists from Table 16: Occupations of the Fathers, Mothers, 85 and Spouses of the Respondents in Science and Other Academic Fields and in Non-Scientific and Non- Academic Occupations Occupation: a)Science and other Academic Fields Scientists in the same field as the Respon— dent Scientist in Different Field from the Respon- dent Academician in Other scholarly Field b)Non—Scientific and Non-Academic Occupations Businessman Clerk Government Service Laborer Lawyer Librarian Physician School Teacher (primary or secondary) Other c)Unemployed Total: Father 7.3% 9.8 1.2 14.6 4.9 20.7 15.9 3.7 6.1 100.0% (N=82) Mother 1.2% 1.2 2.4 7.3 6.1 7.3 63.5 100.0% (N=82) Spouse 7.3% 13.4 6.1 1.2 2.4 6.1 74.5 100.0% (N=82) developi occupati To: respond- discuss of res; What is which e h? 86 developing and developed nations with regard to the occupational backgrounds of their parents and spouses. Turning now to the employment histories of the respondents, the section on dissociative experience discussed briefly foreign work experience. The majority of respondents did not have corss-societal employment. What is of interest here is the type of organization which employed the respondents in their home countries. Table 17: Percentage Distribution of the Type of Organizations Which Employed the Respondents in Thier Home Countries Type of Organization Which Employed the Respondents Per Cent Industry 4.5% Government 11.7 University 78.8 Other 1.4 Unemployed 3.6 Total: 100.0% (N=222) As Table 17 shows, eigth-tenths of the re- spondents worked in a university before their arrival to the United States. Governmental organizations employed one-tenth of the scientists and 4.5 per cent had jobs in industry. There were no major differences between scientists from developed and developing countries. As we have already seen in the section on role circumscrip' forming rese publication tists who we usually stat: schedule the. was Primaril- other hand, identified t In Sump; this study 1 orientations developed an Periences 312' Sitiese Wher P0183 and W? One fur characteris‘ they OQCupy the typ e or 87 circumscription, the majority of scientists were per- forming research, teaching-research, professional, and publication roles in these organizations. Those scien— tists who were employed in industry and government usually stated in response to probes on the interview schedule that the nature of their research involvement was primarily in R & D or applied research. On the other hand, those employed by universities usually identified basic research. In summing the characteristics of the respondents, this study is looking at the behavior, values, and orientations of a younger generation of scientists from developed and developing nations who have limited ex- periences abroad, who are primarily employed by univer- sities, where they enact primarily low circumscribing roles and who do not have a cross-cultural familial and spouse backgrounds. One further take-off point of inquiry into the characteristics of the respondents are the positions they occupy in the work situation in the United States, the type of exchange networks that existed between their national scientific community and the United States, and their experiences in the social system of work in this country. In Chapter 3 the respondents' positions in this country and the exchange networks will be related to a ranking of nations in terms of scientific output and prestige. A discussion of the social system of work in this cou‘ scientists' Chapter 5. 88 in this country as compared to the visiting foreign scientists' home countries will be presented in Chapter 5. Center Chapter 3 Center and Periphery in Science 89, Throug} scientific .2 research at existing pa: in a proces. centers are scientific articles fr presence in In additiOr. their stude‘ SClentists In this way a 'P‘dll " t endeavors. Outsid 01’ 30181193. on here and can DOSSibl Seientlfic is 10,, beca and reSOurC and in term Thus, the PI poveriShed 90 Throughout the world, one can identify centers of scientific activity in a variety of disciplines where research at the forefront of knowledge is transforming existing paradigms or, in some cases, overthrowing them in a process of scientific revolution. Usually these centers are also the major locations of prestigious scientific journals and associations which attract articles from scientists in other countries or their presence in national scientific associational meetings. In addition, scientists outside of the centers send their students to them for socialization under those scientists conducting strategic research in a field. In this way, the centers of scientific activity exert a "pull " towards its vortex of scientific prestigious endeavors. Outside of these centers lie the peripheral areas of science. When major work in a field is being carried on here and, if such work is successful, the periphery can possibly change its position to a focal point of scientific research. But the likelihood of this occurwence is low because the prestige of the centers begets power and resources in the forms of financial and social support and in terms of a flow of highly trained manpower to it. Thus, the peripheral regions of science are often im- poverished and this poverty limits its possible upward mobility in the ranking of nations as centers of scien— tific activity in a field. To study we and perf were as} the fore to disce to the I tions 3i interviI 91 To find out whether or not the scientists in the study were conscious of a distinction between centers and peripheries of research in their field, the respondents were asked to identify the nations in which research at the forefront of their fields is being accomplished and to discern the position of their home country relative to the leaders in their area of scientific inquiry(ques- tions 34, 35, 36, questionnaire and 125, 126, 127, 130 interview schedule). The scientists identify most frequently as centers of research the United States, the Soviet Union, France, Great Britain, and Japan. Furthermore, the United States is, in general, acknowledged as the leader of their fields. The positions of the other four nations usually are seen as being interchangeable in position. As one Canadian scientist pointed out, "except for the United States which is the highest country, there isn't much dif- ference in the nations which are near her." In terms of locating the position of their home countries in relationship to the ranking they presented, the respondents either see their home country as being among or close behind the leading nations or they view it as lagging behind the leading countries in their fields. As Table 18 reveals, scientists from devel— oping nations (high systemic circumscription) usually identify their nations as a peripheral area of scientific activity, i.e., lagging behind the leaders. Scientists Perceiv Positio of the Hone Country 4 Close hind t? 1835.121; 3‘I'c'itions .4 nDOEE C t 92 Table 18: Systemic and Role Circumscription and Perceived Position of the Home Country in Relationship to the Leading Countries in the Respondents Fields Systemic Role Circumscription Circumscription* Perceived Position High (teach- Low (re- of the High Low ing, admin- search, teach— Home (Devel— (Devel- istration, ing-research, Country oping) oped) consulting) Publication, etc) Among or close Be- hind the Leading Nations 36.1% 82.5% 44.3% 72.6% Lagging Be— hind the Leading Nations 63.9 17.5 55.7 27.4 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61 ) (N=137) Q=—.786 Q= ..540 *The total N for Role Circumscription equals 198 and not 222 because of 24 non-responses to the question on role circumscription. from developed nations (low systemic circumscription), on the other hand, rank their home countries as among or close behind the leading nations. Similarly, scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles usually discern the rank of their home country to be lag- ging behind, whereas, those scientists who perform re- search, teaching-research, publication, or professional roles locate the position of their country of origin as among or close behind the top countries in their field. ln,gener role cir of the h tries in 93 In general, therefore, the greater the systemic and role circumscription, the lower the perceived position of the home country in relationship to the leading coun- tries in a field. The respondents were also asked if they thought their national scientific community will be upwardly mobile in the future in terms of their ranking of top countries in their fields. The majority of scientists as Table 19 indicates, express confidence. in the future mobility of their home country. What is interesting, however, is that scientists from developing nations are more likely to acknowledge the possibility of future mobility than scientists from developed nations. There is no relationship between role circumscription (type of work role performed in the home country) and a perception of the future mobility of the home country. Several reasons can be given for the relationship of systemic circumscription (level of development) and perceived future mobility of the home country. Many scientists from developing countries, are conscious of the overall "lowness" of the position of their home coun- try, i.e., they see their country as so far behind the top nations in their fields that downward mobility is an impossibility. As a microbiologist from Greece noted, "my country is so distant from the leaders and so are other nations similar to mine, we can only move up. Down is where we are at.“ For others, there is an inherent Future Mobility of Hone Country Yes No Total *Ihe tot not 222 on role opment ( Curing : 94 Systemic and Role Circumscription and Perceived Future Mobility of the Home Country in Relationship to the Leading Countries in the Respondents Fields Table 19: Systemic Circumscription Future Mobility High Low of Home (Devel- (Devel- Country oping) oped) Yes 72.6% 50.5% N0 270“ 4905 Total 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) Q= .445 High (teach- ing, admin- Role Circumscription* Low (re- search, teach- istration, ing-research, consulting) Publication, etc”) 60.3% 62.2% 3907 3708 100.0% 100.0% (N=61) (N=137) Q= --039 *The total N for role circumscription equals 198 and not 222 because of 24 non-responses to the question on role circumscription. optimism based on the types of advancement in the devel— opment of an adequate scientific community they see oc- curing in the home country. These scientists point out that tremendous strides in self—improvement are underway and a better quality of scientist is becoming predominant in their fields in the home country. Moreover, they often cite increased financial and social support being given to their fields by the public and power centers of their society. It is only a matter of time and the building of a critical mass of scientists for these scientists before their nations narrow the gap between the leading countries and them. Some scientists share this optimism, but temper it with a ( nobility or in the am; 0T prestig. a180 discer a POSitlon their fielfyl taking pla: they Hill r and thEm, y ThOse not see an}, usually ciI students 1: absentee Of of financi and the di in the ho; these SCie is th 11} SCientifi( the dispel: Scie 95 it with a cognizance of the possibilities of future mobility on the part of the top nations. They believe in the ability of their countries to climb the ladder of prestige and power in the scientific community, but also discern the failure of their nations to achieve a position of equal rank with the current leaders in their fields because of the advancements in their fields taking place amongst the top nations. They feel that they will narrow somewhat the gap between the leaders and them, but at the same time acknowledge that the distance will never be fully closed. Those scientists from developing nations who do not see any future mobility of their home countries usually cite the lack of dedicated scientists and students in their fields in the home country, the absence of adequate equipment and facilities, low level of financial and social support given to their work, and the disinterest in creativity in the work situation in the home country as the sources of immobility. For these scientists, the improvement of these conditions is not likely to occur. Rather the “malaise“ of their scientific community will remain and,hence, increase the disparity between their nations and the top countries in their field. Scientists from developed nations, on the other hand, have a lower rate of perceiving future mobility on the part of their home countries than scientists from develc their home For some of in which tr Rather, thj of scientif reference t catch up w: those natic 180k "qual: 337? to Sur the scient: “En thougkl .. ' I tney outlir main stable because Of them. AS C 96 from developing countries because they usually see their home country as among or close behind the leaders. For some of them,they cannot visualize any circumstances in which their country could experience downward mobility. Rather, they see their countries as "permanent" centers of scientific activities in their fields. Others made reference to the inability of nations behind them to catch up with their nations in the ranking system, because those nations who are not of an equivalent or better rank lack "quality" scientists, equipment, and support neces- sary to surpass their home countries. Finally, some of the scientists from developed nations acknowledge that even though their countries are high in the ranking system they outlined, the position of the home country will re— main stable because of no competitors from periphery and because of the further advancement of those nations above them. As one British chemist stated, "Britain will always be number two. The United States and Russia will always be ahead of us, but no nation can take second place from us in the forseeable future." The scientists from developing nations who see their countries as being mobile in the future usually identify the strengths of their national scientific community as the sources of their mobility. Given their strength, they visualize that many of the major break- throughs in their fields will occur in their home country. These successes will enhance the leadership position of their nati: areas of s: as a resulf their home A crif between sci is the imp: scientists the scienti States as T in their f: cmmtries 3 their fieli scientists 97 their nations and increase their "centrality" in their areas of scientific inquiry. Those nations below them, as a result, will not be able to move close to or above their home countries. A critical issue to raise in terms of this difference between scientists in developing and developed nations is the impact of the ranking system of nations on the scientists in this study. As has already pointed out, the scientists in the study usually regard the united States as the center or a center of scientific activity in their fields. Given that scientists from developing countries see their national scientific community in their fields as far behind the leading nations and that scientists from developed nations cast their national scientific community among or close behind the leaders, how does this perspective of the home country's place relative to the United States, affect the scientists in this study? Two areas of possible impact are the positions held by the scientists in the social system of work in the United States and the type of exchange networks which exist between their home country and the United States. Concerning the former, the scientists were asked to identify the type of position they are occupying in the universities during their current stay (questions 6, questionnaire and 7, interview schedule). The positions they hold vary from research assistant to full professor. Generally ' professor ' search ass. are consid- been retai: classified Table 98 Generally the rank of instructorzand assistant to full professor can be regarded as “mainline“ positions. Re- search assistant and associate, on the other hand, usually are considered to be "non-mainline“. This distinction has been retained in the analysis and the respondents have been classified into mainline and non-mainline positions. Table 20: Systemic and Role Circumscription and the Position of the Respondnets in the Social S stem of Work in the United States: ainline vs. Non- Mainline Positions Position of Systemic Role 328t§eifi°tfie Circumscription Circumscriptiigw (re- Social System High (Teach- search, teach- of work in High Low ing, admin- ing-research, the united (Devel- (Devel- istration, professional States oping) oped) consulting) publications) Mainline (Instructor, Assistant, Asso- ciate or Full Professor) 13.8% 30.6% 28.1% 18.3% Non-Mainline (Research As- sistant, Re- search Asso- ciate) 79.2 69.4 71.9 80.3 Non- Response 7.0 ---- ---- ---- Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=1o1) (N=121) ( N=61 ) (N=137) Q= -.4313 Q= +.2651 Furthermore, mainline positions often are regarded as more prestigious than the non-mainline 12Sometimes the position of instructor is regarded in some departments as a non-mainline position. Unfortunately data was not obtained which could determine the rank of instructor in the departments studied. category. r monetary r sitions. for those than for s 13 position. involvenen d8partment POSitions Finally, t varying de the part 0; With Stude: Table to the Dos) of Work in tists fPOm nmkmainli countries. aiministra positions PeSearch’ home count circumscpj mainline I 3651th t} t 99 category. In addition to the prestige factor, is the monetary reimbursement differences between these po- sitions. Usually, the financial rewards are higher for those scientists in instructor to professional rank than for scientists in the research assistant or associate position? Moreover, the mainline positions entail greater involvement in the decision-making structures of the department and greater access to individuals in authority positions in the departments than non-mainline positions. Finally, the difference in positions may also involve varying definitions of roles and social identities on the part of the scientists in terms of their interaction with students and colleagues. Table 20 relates systemic and role circumscription to the positions of the respondents in the social system of work in the United States. Here one finds that scien- tists from developing countries are more likely to hold non-mainline positions than scientists from developed countries. In addition, scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting role hold more mainline positions than scientists who perform research, teaching— research, professional, or publication roles in their home countries. Therefore, the greater the systemic circumscription, the greater the tendency to occupy non- mainline positions in the United States. However, the greater the role circumscription, the greater the tendency to occupy mainline positions. 13In some universities, the position of reserach associate can be quite prestigeful and financially rewarding. Data on such university differences were not gathered. In te viously, t peripheral low status tific ende tists from leaders in will hold i I implies th: PQCEiVe. 1 ranking of the scient. Role 4 OCCupied b} in the Unit Wolved i] roles eanc' °°untry. 1 of team-uni Positions, research. blicatiOn ' 100 In terms of the ranking of nations presented pre- viously, these results imply that scientists from the peripheral ranking countries will occupy positions of a low status in one of the high ranking centers of scien— tific endeavor in their field, the united States. Scien- tists from nations which are close behind or among the leaders in their field when travelling to another center will hold positions which are of a high status. This implies that many of the scientists from developing nations do not receive the same level of reward, prestige, and role involvement as many of the scientists from developed nations receive. Language proficiency may also be a factor. The ranking of the home country, therefore, has an effect on the scientist in the study. Role circumscription has an impact on the position occupied by the scientists in the social system of work in the United States because of the similarity of tasks involved in mainline and non-mainline positions to the roles eancted in the social system of work in the home country. Mainline positions often entail the performance of teaching and administrative roles. The non-mainline positions, on the other hand, are directly related to research, teaching-research, and to some degree pub— blication roles. In other words, there is a continuity of positions for those scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles in their home country and in the United States and also for those scientists who enact research, teaching-research, publication, and profession more eVide The SI periphery 1 do the sci! country 8311' the United if there a country an of informa trarsferenc sponsorhip notificati iability (3 country (QI types of e' iSting bet' sources, CI to this as 101 professional roles. This point will be supported with more evidence in Chapter 5 where it will be shown that the work roles performed in the United States correspond highly with the types of work roles performed in the home country. The second area of interest related to the center- periphery ranking of nations is what type of exchanges do the scientists see as existing between their home country and one of the centers of scientific activity the United States? The respondents were asked to identify if there are any networks of exchanges between their home country and the United States in terms of communication of information in journals and exchanges of journals, tramference of financial and other forms of resources, sponsorhip of students, work contacts with scientists, notification of current news and gossip and of the avail— iability of positions in the United States and the home country (question 39, interview schedule). The major types of exchanges the scientists acknowledge as ex— isting between the two countries are exchanges of re- sources, students, and journals. What is of interest to this discussion is the direction of these exchanges, In An attempt was made also to identify the roles scien- tists enacted in the exchanges between their home country and the United States (question to, interview schedule). four-tenths of the eighty-two scientists gave non— responses to the question and 1.5 per cent said they didn't know if they played any role. As a result, this question did not yield any results which could have given insight into differences between scientists in systemic linkage roles. i.e., are and does p and role c reciprocal a)one way country an to the Uni Table nations ar changes to Oped natio changes as tists fr07 Changes 0:" of the hor from this °f ”Chan: United Ste} Hm Opposil Scierl or consultl of exchan;l 0n the otki by- b -_ 102 i.e., are the exchanges reciprocal or non-reciprocal and does perception of reciprocity vary with the systemic and role circumscription of the scientists? Non— reciprocal exchanges have been divided into two types: a)one way exchanges from the United States to the home country and b)one way exchanges from the home country to the United States. Table 21 indicates that scientists from developing nations are more likely to view the network of ex— changes to be non-reciprocal than scientists from devel- oped nations. The latter scientists regard the ex- changes as being reciprocal. In addition, more scien- tists from developing nations see the direction of ex- changes of a non-reciprocal nature to be flowing out of the home country to the United States rather than from this country to their nation. For those scien- tists from developed nations who see non-reciprocity of exchanges, the one way exchange is initiated by the United States towards the home country rather than in the opposite direction. Scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles also identify non-reciprocal forms of exchanges. Their low role circumscribed counterparts, on the other hand, see reciprocal exchanges existing between the home country and the United States. Of the high role circumscribed scientists, 28.6 per cent see direction of the non—reciprocal networks to be from the Table 21: Types of Exchanges Between th Unitei States and the Home Country Becip- rocal Non-3601p- rocal &)0ne way EXChaRges From U.S, t0 the How Country b)one way ~Cnanges from home Country to the U.s. NOn- ReSDOHSe TOtal Or queStiOn ‘ 103 Table 21: Systemic and Role Circumscription and the Type of Exchanges Between the United States and the Home Countries of the Respondents: Reciprocal and Non-Reciprocal Exchanges gigginggs Systemic Role Circumscription* Circumscription Between the Low (Re- United High (teach- search, teach- States and High Low ing, Admin- ing-research, the Home (Devel- (Devel- istration, Publication, Country oping) eloped) Consulting) Professional) Recip- rocal 50.0% 67.3% h7.6% 73.7% Non-Recip- rocal a)0ne way Exchanges From U.S. to the Home Country 23.5 15.4 28.6 17.5 b)One way Exchanges from Home Country to the U.S. 26.5 11.5 23.8 7.0 Non- Response ---- 3.8 ---- 1.8 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=30) (N=52) (N=21) (N=57) *The N does not equal 82 because of non—responses to question of work roles. United States towards the home country and 23.8 per cent say the exchanges are directed out of their country of origin to the United States. Similarly, 19.7 per cent of these scientists who perform research, teaching— research, publication and professional roles specify one way exchanges from this country to theirs as the direction 8.2 per ce from the i Cauti the small one can cc Peripheral in a field 0? the exc Vise versa 104 direction of the non-reciprocal network, whereas only 8.2 per cent identify an opposite directional flow, from the home country to the United States. Cautiously interpreting these findings because of the small number of scientists involved in this analysis, one can conclude that the systemic linkages between the peripheral nations and the centers of scientific activity in a field are of a non-reciprocal nature and the direction of the exchange is from the center to the periphery or vise versa. Between equal ranked nations, in the leader or close behind the leader, ranks the exchanges are reciprocal, i.e., both nations share in the initiating and receiving of resources, information and students. Two statements made by scientists yield an adequate description of this difference between center and periph- ery in exchange networks. Describing the reciprocity of the systemic linkage exchanges between his country and the United States, a scientist from West Germany said: "every week scientists in my department receive letters from Americans. Sometimes they even call each other on the phone for critical dis- cussions on a research problem. We often tell them of promising students who are interested in their areas and we arrange for these students to work in the United States. The Americans also send students to us. We also receive journals from your country and we send ours to yours. So I must say in answer to your question on direction, it occurs both ways." AChilean 0f exchan. we are a : with us a: United Sta but Aneri< they recef letters tc areas that replies. say 'what' few of on: do not Pet thongh Our cardiOlOgj run into c dynamic PE want me t: water Peg: In s: and thOSe adm inistre Identify J rank ing 0 I I countrieS searCheps ) 3103131 SC ao> IzH ngSpHso unaseuzoz Rm.m: mm.©m mm.mu mm.ms Rm.uo R:.Hu mm.mo pnoeo>ao> 12H Hogan IHSO chase .m.D ca daofim odam Uaoam ogonzomao woman meow xpoz ca mwnapooz pm pqoeo>ao>nH Hm>fipp< Ipso ma xpoz on: no xpoz mxpozpmz oommdnopp< mo mama mo madamz oHQooQ mamoog mamoom on: oaaoom npfisAc npazfio npaznn nuazAm xeo: mo ooampzo mxpozpoz "mxpozpoz HmASpHso chase ho mooHUQH mpnoosommom exp an mxpozpoz HmASpHso usage a“ pmoso>ao>aH no scapsnappmaa owmpsooaom "mm canoe :oo. moo. :Ho.l mom. 500.1 omo.l ANNNuzv .m.D ca Hm>ann< ho 0&5pr woo. omo. NHH. 0H3. mmfi.n moo.u Aodfluzo oaodm ooampso oaaooa suaon omm.u oom.u one. moo. omH. meo.u mam. oofl. ooe.u oom.u 003.: mom.u Aoofiuzo Aoofiuzo cacao oposzomao :H xpoz on: oaaooa oHQooQ sofion opdzfln sHH mam. moo. mom. How. mom.u :H:.I Aoafiuzv woman oemm pm ago: on: oHQooQ nuazfim x903 mo moamuso mxnozpoz Mao. OOH. mmo.n dNHO' omo. mom.l Aofiauzv goo: on mxaozuoz "mxpozpoz Hmthaso chase ca usoso>ao>sH mxpozpoz Homopaso define a“ pcoso>ao>nH op moosoaaomxm o>HpmHoommHQ mo macapmaonnoo Nan. omm. mom. wan. mmm.1 can. Amouzo mwnapooz pm monmocopp< "mm manme muoHpmHOOmm¢ caudpnoaom mmfiopom 2H mmfismponeoz mam nomnoooa so“: :oHpompoch moappssoo mnwmoao>oa on mmdpa mofiapnsoo oomoao>on on moans cocoapomxm xpoz :waopom ooamapoqu Honoapoosom .o.om owfiooom “moozofipomxm o>dpma00mmao 115 First of all, those scientists who have had cross-societal educational and work experiences commonly interact with people from their home country in and outside of the work situation in the United States. Hence, prior work and educational experiences abroad have not stimulated the growth of third cultural networks in the respondents' current trip to the United States. Moreover, cross-societal work experiences have not been of assistance to the respondents in the establish- ment of communication and friendship ties with scientists from other nations at meetings. In contradistinction, educational experiences abroad, along with making trips to developed and developing countries, interacting with foreigners in the home country and belonging to scien— tific associations in other nations have enabled these scientists to use scientific associations as a setting for the development of third cultural social relationships. Secondly, making trips to developed countries appears to be the most functional of the dissociative experiences for the development of third cultural net- works in and out of the work situation and for obtaining sponsorship for the current stay in the United States. This finding indicates that the making of trips to coun- tries has most likely given scientists who have been abroad appropriate experience in establishing social ties with nationals from other countries in the United States. Relying on their cross—societal background, 116 these scientists seek out individuals not from their home country for interaction. The same conclusion can be drawn for those scientists who have been abroad to developing countries with reference to interaction patterns outside of the work situation with people who work at the same place as the respondents and who are in the same field as they are. Additional support is given to this conclusion in the findings presented in Table 8 in Chapter 1. There, educational and work experiences abroad were unrelated to making trips to developed and developing nations. These results indicate that possibly foreign educational and work experience does not give the ex- periental base for participation in third cultural networks as does the foreign experience abroad apart from educational and work purposes. Educational and work experience abroad may in fact not be a stimuli for participation in third cultures at least with reference to third cultural network involvement. Since these experiences were only temporary and time consuming, the respondents may not have been involved with members from other countries and may not have absorbed the cultural patterns which were divergent from their home countries while obtaining their doctor- ate or its equivalent or working. EVen though the trips scientists made to developed and developing coun- tries were also temporary (usually less than a month), 117 the respondents were heavily involved in cross-cultural interaction. The majority of the respondents who made such trips stated the purpose of their journey abroad to be contacting scientists from other nations who were engaged in similar work as theirs. They saw them at conferences or in the scientists' laboratories. In addition, they used such periods to vacation and to see another country. An additional variable of importance is status. During the foreign educationaland work experiences, the scientists are students and do not have established professional identities in the social system of work. However, during their trips to developed and developing countries, they are interacting as colleagues with the scientists they visited, rather than as students with their professors. Hence, the making of trips abroad may be more dissociative than foreign educational and work experiences because of the divergent status positions held by the scientists in each of these experiences. Finally, the relationship among the psychic mobility indices of dissociative experiences and the involvement of the visiting foreign scientists in third cultural net- works ié quite mixed. Interacting with foreigners in the home country is not related to sponsorship of the current trip to the United States nor to participation in third cultural networks in and outside of the work situation. Yet, it is functional for the establishment of third 118 cultural networks at scientific association meetings. Similarly, membership in associations in other countries besides the home country'is functional for third cultural involvement at meetings and also for such involvement with people who work at the same place but outside of the work situation. Respondents who hold such memberships, on the other hand, tend to interact only with people from their home country when their interaction partners work else- where or are in their field. Language facility in English may be a factor here. A lack of facility in English may account for participation with individuals from the home country rather than with a binational and multinational groups. Upon this evidence, one can conclude that only in certain situations does peychic mobility in the home country stimulate involvement in third cultural networks. Post-Modern Orientation. As previously noted, Waisanen has proposed that exposure to dissociative experiences enables a social actor to transcend his social—cultural milieu and through this process develop attitudes Which can be called trans—national. Following this thesis, the first chapter proposed that for the population in this study such dissociative experiences would foster the growth of a "post-modern” orientation. Accordingly, proposition 4 was developed to explore this relation— ship: the greater the exposure to dissociative experiences, the greater the post-modern orientation. “A v.4 “HQ II 1( 119 Questions 67 and 69 in the interview schedule were used to develop a set of fixed alternative ques— tions which could measure post-modern orientations. These questions were incorporated in question 49 on the questionnaire (see Appendix A). Agreement or Dis- agreement with the items in the question were scored as post-modern or non-post-modern in the following manner 3 Statement "My country should stay as it is, ie., it should not change." ”What my country needs most is greater eco- nomic development." ”A greater effort in my home country must be placed on a re- discovery of its past." "The values of science should influence the values and ways of life of the people and leaders of my country.” ”The problems of con— fronting my country must be seen as in- ternational in nature.“ ”My country should follow and develop its own course thru history and not copy other nations." ”There should be more international co- operation between My country and other nations." Agreement Non-Post Modern 15 Post-Modern Non-Post— Modern Post-Modern Post-Modern Non-Post- Modern Post—Modern Disagreement Post-Modern Non—Post- Modern Post—Modern Non-Post- Modern Non-Post- Modern Post-Modern Non-Post Modern 15 Agreement on this item is essentially indicative of a transitional stage prior to the emergence of a post-modern orientation. 120 Following this scoring scheme, Table 24 reveals that the majority of scientists have a post-modern orientation. They view the future of their home coun- try in dynamic terms rather than static and reject a nostalgic perspective for their home country. They also see the future of their home country from an international viewpoint stressing that the problems facing their home country are international in nature. Accordingly, their nation along with other countries cannot develop their own course in history but must work together with a spirit of international coop- eration. These visiting foreign, scientists also emphasize a need for a proliferation of the values of science, rationality and experimentation through the general masses and leadership of their home coun- tries. This type of future orientation is, as described earlier, post-modern. The relationship of post-modern orientation to dissociative experience as formulated in proposition 4 is supported in Table 25. Four of the six indices of level of exposure to dissociative experiences are in the main positively correlated with post-modern orientation items. Work experience abroad, making trips to developed countries, and the two psychic Inobility indices (interacting with foreigners in the luome country and memberships in scientific associations :Ln.other countries) have fostered the growth of post-modern &o.ooa o.m o.oH o.m &m.mm soapmnomooo Hm:0apmmpou InH one: mo.oofi a.m o.mH o.mm Rm.dm mLOpmHm :« ompsoo n30 moao>om HNH mo.ooH mo.oofi Ro.oofi H.m o.m m.s H.afi e.oH n.mm m.oa o.0H H.NN mfi.ao eo.mo no.5: Hmnofip monofiom pmmm mo Imapoan mo hpo>oomaoom one 02Hm> popmoao msoapopm "nowpmpzoapo spooozupmom go mooaonH Aoéfiuzv mzoapmpnoapo apooozlpmomnnoz one mnmoozupmom "mpnooqonmom 0:» mo noapmpnodno onspzm no make mo soapsnahpman owmpqoohom mo.ooH mo.ooa m.e H.m o.n o.m n.0m m.s mfi.ao we.mm unmade mnpmsoo IHo>oQ oeom caeozoom oHnmpm Lopmopo "3m canoe Hoooe monommom Inoz dmdwomdflb apoooz upmomunoz unmoozupmom scapmpnoaao no some NNH oom. mum. omm.1 mun. moo. How. mmm. maoapmfio 10mm< camap unmaom swaopom 2H madnmponao: omfi. oom.- soo.: aofi. HNN. ohm. one. mooomhonoa so“: sofipomuoan Hoo.- com. ooo.u mmm.s Hos. moo.- oom.- moan» 1:500 wnamo IHoboa 0» means dam. one. How.1 moo.l mom. Hofi.1 oom. moan» unsoo oomo IHoboa on agape mom. man. mom. now. own. :oH.I man. oosoapomxm xaoz awaonom mac. oom.: on.u moo.u ono.c mom. non.u ooooahooxm HmnoHpmosom .o.sm omfiopoa nofipmpoaooo encumam a“ ammodpwshop oosoaom pmmm pacemo hhpssou moonoanomxm ammoapwspop owesoo :30 12H can no osam> mo mamp nHo>om oaom o>apmaoommaa IcH mac: moao>on meoanopm lOOmHoom oasoaoom manmpm Lopmoao seasons "sofipmunoapo apooozlpmom mo mooaonH Aoaauzv aofipmpnoapo :LoUOZIpmom mo moofiomH .m> mooaoapomxm o>dpma00mmao "mm canoe 123 orientation. Obtaining a doctorate and its equivalent in another country and making trips to a developing country are in general negatively correlated with this type of perspective on the future. Educational ex- perience abroad is related to a post—modern orientation only with reference to greater economic development. The only index of such a perspective which is related to making trips to developing countries is a rejection of an isolationist view of the historical development of the home country. Table 25 also reveals that those scientists who have been physically and psychically mobile commonly reject the viewpoint that the problems confronting their home country are international in origin. Rather, they usually view the problems of their nation as being in— digeneous to it. This result indicates that exposure to socio-cultural patterns of other nations makes in- dividuals more conscious of the divergence of their own social system from others. Instead of identifying the similarities amongst their system and others, these respondents emphasize the uniqueness of their society relative to others. Such an emphasis implies that these individuals have used other nations as yardsticks of comparison with their home country. This comparative perspective can be characterized as an evaluative ability. Evaluation of one's own system and others is an outcome of dissociative experience and is in keeping with the 124 model that Waisanen has proposed (1969: 7). Since evaluation is a result of exposure to other systems and because only 10.7 per cent of the respondents had non-post—modern orientation on this item, these negative correlations will be considered as supportive of the proposed relationship. In summary, proposition 4, the greater the exposure to dissociative experiences, the greater the post—modernity, has been supported. Those scientists who have been phy- sically and psychically mobile view the future of their home country in a post-modern manner i.e., the physical and social environment of the home country and the world are conquered through the application of science at an international level or the application of international science at the national level. Universalistic Orientations to Work4_Social Interaction and LivinggLocation. Earlier it was suggested that scien- tists who were physically and psychically mobile would be more universalistically oriented than scientists who did not have such an exposure to dissociative experiences. Accordingly, it was proposed the following propositions: Proposition 5: The greater the dissociative experiences, the greater the universalistic orientation to work 0 Proposition 6: The greater the dissociative experiences, the greater the universalistic orientation to social interaction. Proposition 7: The greater the dissociative experiences, the greater the universalistic orientation to living location. 125 Universalistic orientation to work, social interaction, and living location has been defined previously as a lack of boundedness in one social system in terms of where a social actor would work, who he would associate with, or where he would prefer to live. To measure the first item of a universalistic orientation,vnaselected questions 159 on the interview schedule and 40 on the questionnaire. Both of these questions ask the re- spondent if he has a preference to where he works. Similarly, with reference to a universalistic orienta- tion to social interaction and living location, the visiting foreigm.scientists where asked if they had a preference of persons with whom they would interact with (question 147, interview schedule) and a pre- ference on which country or countries they would pos— sibly live in (question 161). Table 26 reveals that the majority of scientists are universalistically oriented with reference to work and social interaction, but particularistically orien- ed to living location. The 74.4 per cent of the re- spondents on the interview who are particularistically oriented made a preference for either their home country or the United States. These respondents selected their home country for several reasons. First and primarily, they had identified their nation as their home society. Moving to another country would entail for them a loss of familial and friendship ties which they valued highly. 126 Table 26: Percentage Distribution of Respondents Type of Orientation to Work, Social Interaction, and Living Location: Universalism and Particularism Type of Social Orientation Work Interaction Living Location Universal- istic 66.7% 54.9% 18.3% Particular- istic 29.3 30.5 74.4 Non- Response 4.0 14.6 7.3 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (n=222) (N=82) (N=82) Table 27: Dissociative Experiences vs. Universalistic Orientation to Work, Social Interaction, and Living Location Type of Universalistic Orientation Dissociative Social Experiences Work Interaction Living Location (N=222) (n=82) (N=82) Foreign Ph.D. Educational Experience -.390 .260 -.356 Foreign Work Experience .307 .030 -.465 Trips to Devel- oped Countries .031 .301 .216 Trips to Devel- oping Countries .091 .359 .247 Interaction with Foreigners .153 -.488 -.383 Memberships in Foreign Scientific Associations .024 .525 -.469 127 Secondly, some respondents felt that they would have difficulty in adjusting to cultural patterns in another country. Moreover, some of these respondents cherished their cultural heritage over the culture of others. They believed their society had something to offer which others did not. Along thse lines, some respondents felt other cultures, specifically the United States, over stressed the Protestant Ethic of work over enjoyment with an overemphasis on work. As one scientist from Australia said, ”I'd rather have a beer with friends and play the horses, than work all the time like these Americans." Others saw their home country as being more rewarding for raising children because from their perspective their culture stressed healthier attitudes toward life, e.g., a lack of stress on violence; or occupational success is not the only important thing in life. Those foreign scientists who made a preference for a nation other than their home country picked some highly developed nation such as the United States, England, or West Germany as their choice of a living location. The major factor at work in this selection was a desire to partake of the riches and comforts of a technological society. Their vision of these countries were similar to the images held by early immigrants to the United States, namely, the highly developed nation is a "land of oppor- tunity," where material and social success is readily possible in one's life time. Some of these respondents 128 united their work location choice with this selection of a residential site. In these more developed coun- tries, they felt they could achieve the best of both worlds: they could find funds and colleagues for work and, at the same time, enjoy the comforts of life. The preference of a highly developed nation for a living location, as can be expected, was usually made by those scientists who came from developing nations. These particularistic orientations to living location are in general associated with an exposure to dissociative experience, as Table 27 reveals. Two of the physical mobility indices, educational and work experience abroad, and both of the psychic mobility indices (interacting with foreigners in the home coun- try and memberships in foreign scientific associations) are negatively correlated with a universalistic orien- tation to living location. Only the making of trips to developed and developing countries in a professional status seem to foster a universalistic orientation, ie., having no preference for a residential site. These results indicate that exposure to dissociative experiences, save for journeys to other nations apart from working and receiving an education, foster an awareness of specific choices of a living location. As in the prior section on post-modernity, this finding suggests an evaluative ability. By becoming conscious of alternatives to the socio-cultural patterns, those scientists who have been 129 exposed to dissociative experiences are selective in identifying the place where they would like to live. Accordingly, proposition 6 should be reformulated in light of this evaluative component: the greater the dissociative experiences, the lesser the universalistic orientation to living location. The relationship of exposure to dissociative ex- periences and a universalistic orientation to social interaction, on the other hand, is supported in Table 2?. Scientists who have had physically and psychically mobile experiences commonly make no preferences by general categories of human identity in their choices of interaction partners in their social life. Most of the respondents felt that cultural differences, political background, race or any other similar categories did not affect their decision as to whom they prefer to or actually interact with. In contrast, proposition 5 is only weakly sup- ported in Table 27. Accepting employment in another country or countries is positively associated with work experiences abroad and interacting with foreigners, not associated with making trips to developed and developing countries and memberships in foreign associations, and negatively correlated with obtaining a doctorate or its equivalent in another country. This weak association of dissociative experience and universalistic orientation to work can be clarified through an examination of Table 28. 130 The respondents were asked on the questionnaire to identify how important or unimportant the following items were in their decisions on where they would work: country, salary, quality of scientists, quality of re- search facilities, and preferences of their wives and/or children (question 41, questionnaire). As Table 28 dis— closes, the majority regard each of these factors to be important in their initial choice. The quality of scien- tists and research facilities are the most important and familial desires the least important. Interview respondents were asked a similar question (question 160, interview schedule) and again a similar distribution emerges. The majority of respondents, 70.6 per cent identified professional factors, such as avail- able funding for research projects, the quality of scien- tists, students, and research facilities, etc. as important in their preference of a country to work in. The next major factor was the country, 18.9 per cent of the in- terview respondents. Idiosyncratic desires, e.g., ability to make friends, personal likes and dislikes, accounted for 5.2 per cent. Familial reasons were given by 3.3 per cent and 2.0 per cent of the responses were uncodable. These results convey that there are definite factors which are incorporated in the decision of a location for employ- ment. The uniformity of selections of these factors by the scientists accounts for the low association between dissociative experience and a universalistic orientation mo.oofi m.mH o.om em.am maaeom Hma mo.ooa mo.ooa mo.ooH mo.ooH o.m o.m m.e m.m a.oH H.NH m.m« o.oH we.ow mo.mm me.oa mo.aa moapaaaomm mpmapnmaom hemamm hppqsoo cosmomom mo mpHHmsd co soflaoso "godpmooq xpoz mo meaozo weapoomo< moapmaam> Aodauzv cosmopOQEH mo Hoboq Edens one oonooooq ago: o oo oohoso moo monsooaoa moapmahm> one no soapsoappmam ommpnoopom "mm canoe Hmpoe omcoamom Inoz unmpLOQEHQD unoppogeH monophomeH mo Ho>oq -‘m “1 “I 132 to work. In summary, exposure to dissociative experiences is related to a universalistic orientation to social interaction and to a particularistic orientation to living location. Its relation to a universalistic orientation to work, however, is weak and is in part attributable to the factors the visiting foreign scien- tists have identified as important in their selection of a locus of employment. Worldmindedness. The relationship of dissociative experiences to this orientation was supported in the section on the post-modern orientation of scientists. In addition, the visiting foreing scientists, in general, stressed an international perspective on the future i.e., international cooperation in solving problems is stressed. This section explores a related issue: the degree of worldmindedness of the respondents and the relation- ship of worldmimiedness to exposure to dissociative experiences as forwarded in proposition 8. As pre— viosuly stated, worldnimdedness is condfiered to be a "world view“ in which the social actor defines his relationship to the world to be characterized by a spirit of cooperation where national differences dis— appear and the world community of man emerges and where cooperation between nations in the solution of common problems takes precedence over an isolationist and na— tionalistic perspective. 133 The respondents were asked in the interview and the questionnaire the open-ended question: "What affect have your experiences here and in other countries had on the way you view people, societies, and the world?” (Qustion 50, questionnaire, and question 234, interview schedule). Several items of information derived from responses to this question are relevant to a consideration of worldmindedness. First of all, did this trip to the United States and prior journeys to other countries have an effect on the world view of the visiting foreign scien- tists? This portion of the data is called "world view change: effect". In this category, responses to the question are coded: 1)a world view change did occur and it was described; 2)a world view change occurred, but no description of the change was given; and 3)no change in the world view was reported. The second item of information concerned the ref- erent of the world view change, i.e., was the world view change either country or transnationally oriented? This information is called "world view change: ref— erent". Responses which were similar to the following were coded as a trans-national orientation: "I have become conscious of the similarities amongst people throughout the world." “The globe seems smaller to me." ”I have found a common humanity to exist and I know we will be able to solve the problems facing us." This referent to the world view change is a country 134 orientation when the scientists made statements similar to these: "I know how far behind our country is from other countries in terms of its progress.“ "I have be- come conscious of the grandeur of the history and culture of my nation.” "After being in the United States, I have seen what the difference is between the 'haves' and the 'have nots'.“ The final piece of data is most relevant to world- mindedness and is entitled “world view change: direction." Here responses to the question were coded with reference to whether or not the change in the world view led to an increase of worldnnmdedness or nationalsim. If the respondent said that as a result of his trips to another nation, he has become knowledgeable of the common humanity of man, or that he now feels that the similarities amongst men are more important than the differences, or that his political, religious, and racial backgrounds no longer interfere from his standpoint in his interaction with people from other nations, I have coded his response as worldmindedness. If on the other hand, his responses were similar to the following I have coded his reply as nationalism: "My nation is more important than others.” "Only people in my home country know how to live." As Tables 29, 30, 31 show, the majority of respondents have recorded changes in their world views as a result of cross-societal movement. Only 7.2 per cent of these scientists could not and did not depict the nature of 135 the change. In describing the referent of the change, 73.5 per cent of the respondents used the world rather than a country as the main point of their discussion. Finally, the impact of cross—societal journeys on the direction of the world view change is reported as being towards worldmindedness rather than nationalism. World view change is, in general, negatively cor— related with exposure to dissociative experiences, i.e., the lesser the exposure to dissociative experiences, the greater the world view change as Table 32 indicates. The implications of this finding is that the current trip to the United States for those scientists who have not been exposed to the various types of dissociative experience has had a greater effect upon them than those who have had such experiences. For these scientists, then their stay has been quite dissociative. This finding implies that the first trip results in greater change than sub— sequent trips. Future trips, therefore, become routinized. The second implication of the results is that scien- tists who have made trips to developing countries and who have interacted with foreigners in the home country have had changes occur in their world views as a result of this trip to the United States and other trips they made abroad prior to their current journey. Changes did not occur in those scientists who have educational and work experiences abroad, made journey to developed countries, and belong to associations in other countries. Concerning "world view change: effect," one must be cautious in arriving at a conclusion with reference to 136 Table 29: Percentage Distribution of World View Change: Effect World View Change: Effect Per Cent Change Described 69.8% Change not Described 7.2 No Change 8.1 Non-Response 14.9 Total 100.0% (N=222) Table 30: Percentage Distribution of World View Change: Referent World View Change: Referent Per Cent Trans-national Orientation 73.5% Country Orientation 25.8 Non-Response .7 Total 100.0% (N=155)* *The N does not equal 222 because of the respondents who did not describe the change in their world views --16, who did not have any change --18, and the 33 who did not respond to the question. Table 31: Percentage Distribution of World View Change: Direction World View Change: Direction Per Cent Worldmindedness 67.7% Nationalism 25.8 Non-Response 7.1 Total 100.0% (N=155)* 'The N does not equal 222 because of the respondents who did not describe the change in their world views--16, who did not have any change--18, and the 33 who did not respond to the question. 137 the relationship of dissociative experience to it because only 8.1 per cent of the respondents identified no change as occuring. A transnational referent, however, is;also associated with exposure to dissociative experiences. Those scien— tists who have not had these experiences cast their description of the type of effect trips to other nations have on them in terms of a country referent, i.e., their orientation identifies a change in attitude toward a particular country, its people, and its culture. Those scientists who have been exposed to dissociative ex- periences, on the other hand, use a trans-national orientation to describe the changes in their world view, i.e., they have become conscious of the similar- ities of people throughout the world. In addition, exposure to dissociative experiences is weakly correlated in a positive direction with a growth of worldmindedness rather than nationalism. Those scientists who have such experiences say that they have become more conscious of the international and trans- cultural nature of the world and their place in it. Scientists who have not had dissociative experiences, on the other hand, see the world in an opposite manner. Considering the positive correlations between dissociative experience and world view change referent and direction together, one can conclude that proposition 8 has been substantiated: the greater the dissociative 138 Table 32: Dissociative Experiences vs. Worldmindedness: World View Change Effect, Referent and Direction World View Change: Dissociative Effect Referent Direction Experiences (N=222) (N=155) (N=155) Foreign Ph.D. Educational Experience -.277 .230 .099 Foreign Work Experience -.292 -.120 -.300 Trips to Devel— oped Countries -.233 .355 .249 Trips to Devel- oping Countries .260 .177 .091 Interaction With Foreigners .212 .362 -.080 Memberships in Foreign Scien- tific Asso- ciations -.448 .364 .504 experience, the greater the worldmindedness. Social and Scientific Responsibility.. In Chapter 1, several repercussions of systemic and role circum- scription were analyzed at a theoretical level. One of the effects of being system and role bounded was identified as a greater commitment to social respon- sibility, i.e., an awareness and acknowledgment of the interdependent relationship of science and society. Two dimensions to social responsibility were indicated. The first is a societal dimension in which the scientist 139 is concerned with the possible consequences of his work on his society and the world. The second dimen- sion is a scientific responsibility towards the scien— tific community in terms of a commitment to the train— ing of future generations of scientists. In light of this discussion, the following proposition was outlined: The greater the systemic and role circum— scription, the greater the social responsibility at the societal and scientific levels. To measure the first dimension of social respon- sibility, scientists were asked: 1)if they felt a sense of responsibility for the possible social con- sequences of their research (questions 29, questionnaire and 76, interview schedule), 2)if they approved of the involvement of scientists in national decision making (question 31, questionnaire), and 3)if they determined their choice of a research topic on the basis of problems facing mankind, or facing their home country, or scien- tific problems (question 27, questionnaire). The scien— tific dimension of social responsibility is measured by the questions which asked if the scientists felt they had an obligation tx>the training of future gener- ations of scientists (questions 19, questionnaire and 195, interview schedule) and why they felt obligated (question 196, interview schedule). In addition, if the scientists chose research topics as the basis of scientific problems rather than problems facing mankind 140 or their home country, this choice will be considered as a measure of scientific social responsibility. Table 33: Percentage Distribution of the Indices of Social Responsibility at the Societal Level and Type of Responsibility Indices of Societal Social Responsibility: Attitude Toward Choosing Choosing Perceived Scien— Problems Problems Type of Soc. Bes- tists in Relevant Relevant Respon— ponsibil Decision to Man- to Home sibility: ity Making kind Country Social Respon- . sibility 85.6% 53.6% 70.0% 67.9% No Respon- sibility 10.8 24.3 25.0 26.4 Un- decided ---- 22.1 ---- -_-- No Res- ponse 3.6 1.0 5.0 5.7 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=222) (N=140) (N=140) (N=140) The majority of scientists, as Table 33 reveals, feel socially responsible at the societal level: 85.6 per cent feel a sense of responsibility for the pos- sible social consequences of their work, 53.6 per cent approve of the involvement of scientists in national decision-making structures, and 70 per cent and 67.9 per cent respectively say that the problems facing mankind and their home country are important in their selection of a research topic. Again there is a striking 141 uniformity of attitudes shared by these scientists. Even though there is this uniformity of a belief in social responsibility, these scientists do not act out this belief except with referenc?6to factors influencing a choice of research problems. When they were asked if they belong to any organizations which promote an awareness amongst scientists of their possible social responsibility, e.g., the Bulletin of Atomic Scientists, only seven of them hold such memberships. Moreover, the scientists were asked if they were involved in any way in changing their home country. Only eight said they had acted as change agents. When asked why they were not involved in changing their society, the replied were either “I'm too involved in my work to bother with it,“ or "It's outside my role as a scientist." In short, social responsibility at the societal level is an attitudinal rather than a behavioral norm for these scientists, except with reference to factors affecting the choice of research topics. jbln the next section the non-professional participation of these scientists will be examined and the results will show that the majority of scientists do not participate in extra—scientific affairs. This non—participation and the lack of a behavioral dimension to social respon- sibility indicate that the visiting foreign scientists can be considered I’social celibates.“ All of their act- ivities are oriented around the scientific community and this orientation requires a large investment of time and energy. Involvement in extra-scientific affairs at the community and national level would detract from their investment capabilities in the scientific community in (continued on next page) 142 Table 34: Percentage Distribution of Social Responsibility at the Scientific Level: Obligation to Next Generation of Scientists Obligation to Next Generation of Scientists Per Cent Yes 85.2% No 7.2 Non-Response 7.6 Total . 100.0% (N=222) Table 35: Percentage Distribution of Social Responsibility at the Scientific Level: National and International Obligation to Next Generation of Scientists Type of Obligation to Next Generation of Scientists Per Cent National 23.2% International 52.4 Don't Know 4.9 Non-Response 19.5 Total 100.0% (N=82 ) , ra5fltragga-frum-grevtous—paga7 1.é’terms of researc product v ty as PPObeS on these questions indicated. By restricting their activities to science, they feel they increase their productivity level. Moreover, as the above section reported, many scientists feel extra-scientific involvement is outside of their role as a scientist. In this way, the lack of participation in activities outside of the scientific com— munity resembles the function of celibacy in the Catholic Church at an "ideal" level where priests restrict their duties to their roles as a priest by avoiding participation in outside organizations and marriage, having more time to devote to their priestly functions. 143 Table 36: Percentage Distribution of Social Responsibility at the Scientific Level: Level of Importance of Scien— tific Problems as a Factor in In— fluencing Choice of Research Topics Level of Importance of Scientific Problems as a Factor Influencing Choice of Research Problems: Per Cent Important 91.5% Unimportant 7.3 Undecided --_- Non-Response 1.2 Total 100.0% (N=140) On the scientific dimension to social respon- sibility, Tables 34, 35, and 36 indicate that most of the scientists used scientific problems as a criterixlfor the selection of a research topic and expressed an obligation to the next generation of scientists. When asked why they had this obligation, two major types of responses emerged. The majority said they are engaged in or would engage in training a future generation of scientists because of a com— mitment to the growth of the international community of scientists. A minority said that they were obligated because they needed to build up science in their home country. The former is called in Table 35, an in- ternational obligation and the latter a national obliga- tion. 144 Table 37: Systemic and Role Circumscription vs. Societal and Scientific Social Respon- sibility Type of Circumscription: Type of Responsibility Systemic Role a) Societal Perceived Social Responsibility (N=222) .492 .150 Attitude Toward Scientists in Decision Making (N=140) .265 .13? Choosing Problems Relevant to Mankind (N=140) -.090 -.300 Choosing Problems Relevant to Home Country (N=140) .404 .137 b) Scientific Obligation to Next Generation (N=222) -.136 .187 Type of Obligation to Next Generation (N=82) .258 —.015 Choosing Problems Relevant to Scientific Problems (N=140) .450 -.675 The national obligation to the next generation of scientists from developing nations adds further insight into the differences between the peripheral ranking of their home countries and those nations who are ranked as centers of scientific activity presented in Chapter 3. An emphasis on building the national scientific community in one's field, rather than the international scientific community, is one way of achieving mobility in the stratification system. A stress on strength— ening of science throughout the world by scientists from 145 developed nations may be considered as a new form of colonialism. Because those outside of the center have relatively little chance to improve their position, the growth of viable research institutes in the per- ipheral nations will provide more scientists for the centers and thus increase the dominance of the centers over the periphery. The low correlation of role circumscription to type of obligation to the next generation indicates that the performance of varying types of high and low role circumscribing activities to an international or national commitment is unrelated. It is the level of educational and scientific development of the country of origin, then, which is crucial in determining the form of an obligation to the next generation. Because of the very smallinmmer of scientists who were uncommitted to future generations, and who did not select scientific problems as a criteria for a choice of research problems, only an implication of the direction of the relationships amongst systemic and role circumscription and scientific social respon- sibility can be given. The slight negative correlation of systemic circumscription to obligation to the next generation indicated that scientists from developed nations may be more socially responsible than scien- tists from developing nations on this index. The latter, however, are more likely to use scientific 146 problems as a criteria for problem selection. Con- cerning role circumscription, scientists who perform teaching, administrative or consulting roles are more obligated to the future generation, but less prone to use scientific problems for selecting a research topic than scientists who perform research, teaching-re— search, professional, or publication roles. The re- sults, therefore, are inconclusive concerning the pro- posed relationship of systemic and role circumscription to the scientific level of social responsibility as stated in proposition 9. Proposition 9, however, is supported in ref- erence to societal social responsibility. Systemic and role circumscription is positively correlated with all the indices, except for the use of problems facing mankind. Here the slight negative correlation of systemic circumscription and the negative correlation of role circumscription to this index reveal that scientists from developed nations and scientists who perform low circumscribing roles claim more global ramifications for their research than scientists from developing nations and scientists who perform high circumscribing roles. The latter, on the other hand, are more conscious of problems facing their home coun- try as previously noted. Proposition 9, then, can be accepted with reference to societal social respon- sibility and is inconclusive with reference to scien- tific social responsibility. 147 Professional and Non-Professional Participation. Propositions 10 and 11 explore issues related to societal and scientific social responsibility. The first attempts to find out whether or not scientists who are systemically and role circumscribed in their systems participated more in extra—scientific activities and affairs than those scientists who are not bounded. The second implies a reverse relationship, i.e., those scientists who are not circumscribed in their system and the roles they perform have higher rates of par- ticipation in scientific activities than scientists who are systemically and role bounded. To measure participation in non-professional activities, the visiting foreignlscientists were asked if they are or have been members of any civic, charitable, relig— ious, political, and non-professional organizations in their country (question 48, questionnaire and 205, interview schedule). As Table 38 indicates, the majority are not and/or were not involved in such activities and only a few are participating in and/or were participating in non—professional organizations. Most scientists in the study, therefore, are not in non-professional organizations in their own country. This finding implies that the scientists restrict theirorganized social life to the scientific community. Table 38 also includes the correlations of non- professional participation to systemic and role circum- scription. Systemic circumscription is weakly positively 148 correlated with it. This result reveals that the development of the home country educational and scien- tific institutions and the types of roles performed by scientists do not influence their non-professional participation. Rather lack of participation in these activities appears to be a characteristic of the gene- ral behavior of scientists in the scientific community. Hence, proposition 10 has not been supported by the data. The extent of professional participation of these scientists was determined by asking the respondents the following: how frequently they attended national scien- tific meetings, how many books they have published, and how many papers and articles they have published. With regard to the attendance at scientific asso- ciational meetings, scientists were asked if they attended every meeting, most meetings, some meetings, and none of the meetings. Attendance at every meeting was given a score of 3, most meetings 2, some meetings 1, no meetings 0. The mean score of the scientists in the study, 1.67, is used to determine level of pro— fessional participation on this measure. Those who scored above the mean are considered as having a high professional participation; those below the mean, low professional participation. The mean rate of paper and book publication is also used to categorize scientists as high or low. 149 Table 38: Percentage Distribution and Correlations of Non—Professional Participation to Systemic and Role Circumscription Systemic Role Circumscription Circumscription* Low (Research, Non—Pro- High (Teach- Teaching-Re— fessional High Low ing, Admini- search, Pro- Partici- (Devel- (Devel- stration, fessional, pation oping) oped) Consulting) Publication) Yes 31.7% 33.9% 37.7% 33.6% No 61.4 64.5 59.0 63.5 Non- Response 6.9 1.6 3.3 2.9 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61) (N=137) *The total N for Role Circumscription equals 198 because of 24 non-responses to the question on role circumscription. Publication of eleven or more papers and articles was given a score of 3, six to ten 2, one to five 1, no articles or papers 0. The publication of seven or more books was scored as 3, four to six books 2, one to three books 1, and no books as 0. Scientists are considered as being high in professional participation if they published.one or more books (mean is .28 for book publication in sample) or two or more papers or“ articles (mean is 1.44 for paper and article publica— tion). 150 Table 39: Percentage Distribution of Level of Professional Participation Indices: Attendance at Meetings, Book Publication, and Paper Publication Indices of Professional Participation: Level of Pro- fessional Attendance Partici- at Book Paper pation Meetings Publication Publication High 40.7% 17.6% 41.0% Low 39.2 82.4 46.4 Non— Response 20.1 ---- 12.6 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=222) (N=222) (N=222) Mean: 1.67 .28 1.44 As Table 39 records, the majority of scientists have a high level of professional participation in attendance at scientific meetings. Most of them, 82.4 per cent, however, have not published any books. In terms of paper publication, 41.0 per cent of the visiting foreign scientists have published six or more articles. Therefore, the scientists in this study are not high producers. Systemic and role circumscription are in general negatively correlated with professional participation. Table 40 reveals that scientists from developed nations have higher level of attendance at meetings and paper publication and a greater book publication rate than 151 scientists from developing nations. Similarly, scien- tists who perform roles which have been described as low circumscribing role have higher rates of attendance at meetings and paper and article publication than scien- tists who perform high circumscribing roles. The latter, however, tend to have a higher rate of book publication. These findings support the relationship of systemic and role circumscription as described in proposition 11: The lesser the systemic and role circumscription, the greater the professional productivity and participation. Table 40: Systemic and Role Circumscription vs. Professional Participation Type of Circumscription Type of Professional Participation Systemic Role (N=222) (N=198) Attendance at Meetings -.286 -.337 Book Publication -.O75 .161 Paper Publication -.142 -.218 In summary, proposition 10 has not been sub— stantiated. Systemic and role circumscription are not related to participation in activities and organ- izations outside of the scientific community. They are, however, correlated with the indices of prof- essional participation, affirming the hypothesis that the higher the systemic and role circumscription, the 152 lower the professional participation. Level of development of the home country and the types of work roles performed in home country, then, affect the respondents' participation in the international scientific community, but do not affect their partici- pation in social activities outside of science. Chapter 5 Further Exploration: Differences Between Social Systems of Work in the United States and the Home Country 153 154 Dissociative Experiences has been defined pre- viously in Chapter 1 as the movement from one social system to another and as exposure to the ideas, values, and members from another social system. In this section, the point of departure rests on the following question: Is the current stay of these visiting foreign scien- tists a movement from one social system of work to another or is the current cross-societal work ex- periences identical to the work experiences of these scientists in their current country? The emphasis here is on whether or not the current stay in the United States is a dissociative experience. In order to answer this query, respondents were asked to compare their work experiences in the United States to those they had in their home country along several dimensions: 1)differences between American and home country students (Questions 20, questionnaire and 42, 43, interview schedule), 2)differences in the relations they had with persons in authority positions in the work situa— tion (questions 21, questionnaire and 44, 45, inter- view schedule), 3)differences between their American and home country colleagues (question 25, questionnaire), 4)differences in their work involvement in the United States and the home country (question 22, questionnaire and 52, interview schedule), and 5)differences in the type of work roles they performed in the United States and the home country (question 17, questionnaire and 155 30 and 31 interview schedule). Differences Between Students. Comparing students in the home country to those in the United States, the majority of visiting foreign scientists see a dif- ference between the students as Table 41 shows. This table also discloses the relationship of systemic circum- scription (level of development) and role circumscription (type of work role performed in the home country) to acknowledgement of a difference between students. The results imply that scientists from developed countries are more likely to identify a difference between American and their home country students than scientists from developing countries. Type of work role performed in the home country (role circumscription) is unrelated to perceiving such a difference. The respondents were also asked to specify what type or types of differences there were between their home country and American students. Three major types emerged from the analysis of responses. First of all, American students and home country students differ in terms of the type of professor—student relationships in the work situation. For some scientists, the in- teraction patterns between student and professor were more collegial in the United States than in the home country, i.e., professors and students treat each other as colleagues rather than in terms of a super- ordinate-subordinate authority relationship. For 156 Table 41: Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences Between the Social Systems of Work in the Home Country and the United States: Differences in Students Differ- ences Between Systemic Role Social Circumscription Circumscription* Systems of Work: High (Teach- Low (Research, Student High Low ing, Admini- Teaching-Re— Differ— (Devel- (Devel- stration, search, Publi— ences oping) oped) Consulting) cation, etc.) Yes 65.3% 63.6% 60.7% 65.0% No 22.8 16.5 21.3 19.? Non- Response 11.9 19.9 18.0 15.3 Total 100.0%’ 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61 ) (N=137) Q= --438 Q: -.0732 *The total N for Role Circumscription equals 198 and not 222 because of 24 non-responses to the question on role circumscription. other respondents, the opposite is true. Secondly other scientists mention that the students differ in the breadth of knowledge they have of the field, i.e., the extent of their knowledge of the theory and techniques of their respective fields. Some scientists said their home country students had a broader knowledge of their fields than did American students. Incontradistinction, other scientists indicated that American students had a better grasp of the theoretical and methodological issues of their fields than their home country students. This point will be elaborated later in this chapter. 157 The third type of difference between students identified by a portion of the respondents is based on the divergent work habits of American and Home country students. Either American students were seen as working harder than their counterparts in the home country or home country students were viewed as expending more effort in their work than American students. The N's in Tables 42, 43, and 44 show that more scientists compared the students in terms of the type of professor—student relationship (N=64) than with reference to the breadth of knowledge of the field possessed by students (N=49) and the work habits of the students (N=45). The interaction patterns between students and professors are generally more salient to the scientists than the ”quality" of the student in comparing students in the home country to students in the united States. With reference to students, then, the differences between the social systems of work is primarily structural. Tables 42, 43, and 44 also portray the relation- ship of systemic and role circumscription to these three types. Scientists from developing nations are more likely to view the professor-student relationship in the home country as being collegial in comparison to such relation- ships in the United States than scientists from devel— oped nations. Moreover, for scientists from developing oma doo. no omm.1 ud Ammuzo Afimuzo Ammuzo Aamuzo mo.ooH mo.oofi mo.oofi mo.oofi Hence o.:o o.ao o.mm o.Hm mapssoo osom 03p :a mQaSmmoapmHom osmosumupommomoam Hmfiwoflaoo &H.mm ma.om Re.me mo.om .m.D on» Ga maasmsoaumaom uncoopmuaommomonm Hmawoaaoo A.opo .soapmo AmmapHSmsoo Aooaoao>oav AmmaQOHo>omo aspnsoo 0803 one .m.D esp ndaosm .sopmom .QOAomapmasaeoa son swam :« moanmsoapmaom uncoopw .1omuwsdnomoe .wsasomoev swam Inommomopm Hmawoaaoo soamomomo son :0 no Useem masoUSpm Humanomssopao sowpaapomesoawo oHom oesopmzm zoozuom moowopomoan Aaonzv moanmaoapoaom uncoopwuLOmmomopm Hmawoafloo so oommm moonoaom Imam "mucoUSpm zpossoo osom one smeaaos< soozpom neoconommfim one scapaanomesoaao oHom use oasopmmm "we canoe omfi Hoo. no mom. no Ammuzv Aoanzv Ammnzo Aamuzv mo.ooH mo.ooH mo.ooH mo.ooa Hence H.mm o.o: m.oo o.om mpsooSpm mwpqaoo oaom can» odoam mo swooazosm hoomoam m obmm mpsooSpm haunzoo oaom mo.od mo.oo mn.om me.Hm mpaooSpm Kpsegoo oeom ammo cacao mo swooazomx godmonm m obs: monoUSpm smoapos< A.ouo .soHumo AmmapHszoo Aooaoaosoav Ammaaoaopoov damam mo swooa IHHnsm .xopmom .aowpmapmanaso¢ 309 swam Izonx mo :pommpm so oommm nomuwcasomoa .wsazomoev swam mpsoozpm hpussoo meow one .Soamomomv 30A smodaos< soozpom moononommfio soapaaaomssoaflo oaom :ofipaanomESopHo ofisoumzm Amanzv wanna go swooazosx monoUSpm mo noomopm so ommmm moosopom than "monoUSum zapssoo oeom one smoagos¢ :oozpom moosopommaa one scapaasomssopao oaom one ofiempmmm "no manna 00H amm.a no mmH.: no Ammuzo Aaauzo Ammnzo A mmuzv mo.ooH Ro.oofi mo.oofi mo.oofi Hmpoe “.mm m.ma o.am 0.0a mosoozam sooasosa can» poopmm xmoz mucooSpm zapqsoo meow ao.no mm.:m Rm.mo ma.mm monoosom hapszoo osom amen Loopmm ago: wpsoospm smoapms< n.0uo .soHpmoHHnom AwsHpHSmsoo Aooaoao>omv Ammam0H0>0Qv mpapmm ago: so oommm noamommmuwnasomoe soHpmnpmasasoa 30A swam musoospm zapnsoo oeom dam .nopmomomv 30a .wmazomoev swam smoapoe< noozpom mooaopohmaa scapaaaomasopwo oaom scaumanomESopHo owsopmmm Amauzv mpsoosom go moans: ago: so oommm moomopom Imam "noncompm appszoo oeom use smoanoe< moospom moososommdo one scapaapomssondo oaom use oasoummm "a: edema 161 nations, American students have a broader knowledge of the field than the students in their home country. Scien- tists from developed nations, on the other hand, believe that their home country students' knowledge of the theo— retical and methodological issues confronting the field is greater than students in the United States. Systemic circumscription (level of development) is unrelated to perceived differences between students based on work habits. Role circumscription is unrelated hoperceived differences between students based on collegial professor-student relationships and breadth of knowledge. Type of work role performed in the home country is however, related to differences based on the work habits of students. Those scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles (high role circumscription) see their home country students expending more effort in their work than students in the United States. Scien- tists who perform research, teaching-research, scien- tific publication writing, or participate in the pro- fessional activities in science (low role circumscription) view the American student as working harder than the home country student. The general implications of these findings is that the scientist from developing countries has a greater adjustment to make in moving ix) the social system of work in the United States, with reference to the students he works with. From this perspective, the students he 162 interacts with in this country have a broader know— ledge of the field he is in and, as a result,they may be more challenging to him. Moreover, he has to con— front a new set of interaction patterns between student and professor. In the United States, these patterns are for him less collegial than in his home country. As a result, he has to redefine his role in relation- ship to the students he works with. In his home coun- try, his role is that of a senior colleague and his students are junior colleagues. But in the United States, this relationship is of a superordinate- subordinate form and requires a transformation of the definition of his role and that of his students. For the scientists from developed nations such role re- definitions and possible challenges from students are less likely to occur. Similarly, teachers, administrators, or consultants may find the increased work effort of students in the United States demanding and/or rewarding. American students by working harder possibly complete projects assigned to them quicker than these scientists haVe been used to in their home countries. As a result, the scientists are required to find additional work for the students who work under them. The search for the projects may place a strain on the scientist toward more output than he has experienced with home country students. But the work habits of the American student may also be rewarding to the scientists in the sense that 163 his work assigned to students arrives at completion faster than in the home country. Therefore, he can be more flexible in the directions his research takes him. In either case, the increased work effort of American students may also entail a redefinition of the roles of the scholar and his student as mentioned above with reference to the professor-student relation- ship. Such a role redefinition on the part of teachers, administrators, and consultants could occur for another reason. The roles they performed in the home country were not of the research type, as we shall see later in this chapter. In the United States, they are performing, for the most part, research roles. Their patterns of in- teraction in the home country with students would be in terms of the roles they had performed. But in the United States, the roles they are enacting are of a different type and, hence, require a different definition of their role and the role of the student. Thus, the transition in the type of roles they perform may be an additional source of accommodation to the social system of work. Beyond the question of the adjustment of scientists to the students in the United States, one has to ask why did these scientists regard collegial relationships be- tween student and professor to be more prevalent in their home country than in the United States? In probing on the differences between students, several scientists from 164 developed and developing countries outlined the struc- ture of interaction patterns between students and fa— culty in the United States in terms of a hierarchy of quality of these patterns. They stated that undergra- duate students working in the laboratories are viewed by the faculty as embryonic technicians who are assigned menial research tasks. The role of the faculty member with relationship to the undergraduate student is that of a manager of research technicians. Most of the in- teraction which occurs between the two is limited to suggestions on how to improve a particular laboratory process or technique. Only the brightest of these stu- dents are welcomed to explore theoretical issues with the faculty member. Graduate students are above the undergraduates in the hierarchy. They are assigned tasks more central to the research endeavor in terms of theory and less in terms of technique. The faculty member here is again viewed as the manager of the research enterprise and his graduate students his employees. As with the undergraduate students, only the brightest of the graduate students are allowed to have more intimate and frequent contact with the faculty member in discussions of the theoretical issues of the research project. Above these students are the post-doctorates who are in collegial relationships with the permanent members of the faculty. The post-doctorates are usually the 165 scientists who have most contact with the graduate and undergraduate students. They oversee the research pro- ject and guide the students in their work. The permanent faculty members manage the financial sides of the project and have frequent conversations with the post—doctorates on the theoretical and methodological problems and suc- cesses of the research project. It is only in the inter- action relationships between permanent faculty and the post-doctorates that collegial relationships are dominant. In their home countries, these scientists stated that both undergraduate and graduate students are treated more as junior colleagues and less as technicians. More frequent discussions and debates over the theoretical im— plications of research occur between them than between students and faculty in the United States. This relation— ship is viewed by many of the scientists as a master- apprentice relationship. For them, the social system of work in the United States resembles, as one scientist stated, a "factory'I which stresses efficiency and effec- tiveness of a research project over the benefits students can derive from participation in the project. However,as we shall see shortly, the relationships of faculty members to each other in the home country are less likely to be collegial as they are in the United States and more likely to be of a superordinate-subordinate type. The system of professor-student relationships in the United States may explain why these scientists identified 166 American students as working harder than their home country counterparts. In a rigid hierarchical system where there seems to be a pecking order of students, undergraduate and graduate students are driven to in- crease their work effort in order that- they may enter into a more meaningful set of relationships with facul- ty members. For example, one scientist from Australia mentioned that there are frequent colloquia in which famous scholars are invited to discuss problems at the threshold of a field. After the scholar makes his pre- sentation, informal conversations are held. All stu- dents receive invitations to hear the speaker, but only those who are acknowledged as being "good" and "bright" students are allowed to partake in these in— formal discussions. Hence a student must work hard to surpass his colleagues to receive such a reward. Several scientists from developing nations offered another reason why collegial professor—student relation- ships are more prevalent in the home country than in the United States. For many of them, the only colleagues they have are their students. The absence of other faculty mem- bers to discuss issues central to their fields requires a greater reliance upon students. They are, in some cases, the only audience the scholar has to discuss with, criti- cize, and evaluate his ideas. In the United States, facul- ty members provide this functbn for the scientists and the result is a decrease, from their perspective, in frequency 167 of communication with students and a change in the quality of interaction patterns between students and professors. Some scientists from developing nations also ex— plained why they regarded their students as working harder than American students on the basis of the facili- ties available to the student. In the home country, equipment and technology available for carrying on re- search is limited. In addition, the libraries students have access to are inadequate. As a result, their stu- dents must work harder to fulfill the same task that American students may be doing. The level of develop- ment of the scientific community in the home country, therefore, is critical in understanding the differences between students in the United States and the home coun- try. In summary, for the majority in this study there is a difference between the social systems of work in the home country and the Uhited States with reference to stu- dents. The scientists are not moving across societal boundaries into a system identical to their own, but are moving into a work situation which is divergent from their own. The difference between the systems is greater for scientists from developing nations (high systemic circumscription) and less for scientists from developed nations (low systemic circumscription). Bole circumscrip- tion (type of work role performed in the home country) 168 is important for the differences between social systems only in terms of differences in the work habits of students. Differences in Authority Relations. Another important aspect of the movement of scientists from one social system of work to another is the type of relationships the scientists have to persons in positions of authority in the work situation, e.g., department chairmen, deans, administrators, etc. To determine whether or not the interaction patterns scientists had with such indivi— duals is different in the United States as compared to the home country, respondents were asked the fol- lowing question: ”Do you find that there is a dif- ference in the way you interact with persons in authority here (e.g., department chairmen, deans, etc.) as com— pared with the way you interacted with similar indivi— duals back home?" (question 21, questionnaire and 4# and #5 interview schedule). Table 45 presents the re- sults and the relationship of the perception of dif- ferences to systemic and role circumscription. The majority of scientists in the study acknowledge a difference in the way they interact with persons in authority positions in the work situation in the United States and in the home country. Both systemic and role circumscription are related to the perception of dif- ferences in the social system of work. Scientists from developing countries perceive a difference to a slightly 169 Table 45: Systemic and Role Circumscription and the Perception of Differences Between the Social Systems of Work in the Home Country and the United States: Dif- ferences in Relations with Persons in Authority Positions in the Work Situation Perception of Dif- ferences Between Systemic Bole Social Circumscription Circumscription* Systems: High (Teach- Low (Research, Authority High Low ing, Admini- Teaching-Re- Relation (Devel- (Devel- stration, search, Publi— Differences oping) oped) Consulting) cation, etc.) Yes 58.u% 50.4% 55.7% 51.1% No 32.7 38.8 29.5 40.9 Non-Response 8.9 10.8 1h.8 8.0 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61 ) (N=137) Q: 0155 Q: 0201" *The total N for Role Circumscription equals 198 and not 222 because of 2h non-responses to the question on role circumscription. greater degree than scientists from developed countries. Similarly, scientists who have performed teaching, admini— strative, or consulting roles also have a greater fre- quency of noting differing authority relations in the United States than scientists who enact research,teach- ing-research, and other low circumscribing roles. When asked to identify the nature of the dif— ference in interaction patterns with persons in the authority positions, the respondents noted only one 170 type of difference: either the authority relations in the home country are less collegial than in the United States or the authority relations in the home country are more collegial than in the United States. No ref- erence to the quality or characteristics of the persons occupying positions of authority in the work situation was made by the scientists. The major difference between the United States and the home country in terms of the relationship of the scientists to persons in authority positions is, then, structural. As Table 46 shows, systemic circumscription is related to the viewing the nature of the difference between authority relations in terms of the degree of collegiality of interaction patterns with persons in authority position in the work situations of the United States and the home country, but role circumscription is unrelated to it. That is, scientists from developing countries see the authority relations in the home coun- try as less collegial than in the United States, whereas scientists from developed countries are more likely to view them as more collegial in the home country than in the United States. Table #6 also reveals that the majority of scientists regard their interaction patterns with dep- artment chairmen, deans, etc. to be less collegial in the home country than in the United States. In the interviews, scientists were asked why the degree of collegiality varied in both societies. Most 171 Table 46: Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences of Authority Relations in the United States and the Home Country: Degree of Collegiality of Relationships with Persons in Authority Positions in the Work Situation.* Differences Systemic Role g£i€;t2;la_ Circumscription Circumscription tionships High (Teach— Low (Research, in the U.S. High Low ing, Admini- Teaching—Re- and Home (Devel- (Devel- stration, search, Publi— Country oping oped) Consulting) cation, etc.) Authority Relations in the Home Coun- try are less Collegial than in the United States 67.0% 51.1% 53.1% 56.8% Authority Relations in the Home Coun- try are more Collegial than in the United States 33.0 49.9 46.9 43.2 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=78) (N=90) (N=49) (N=102) Q: .351 Q: .077 *The N's do not equal 222 because of non-responses and those respondents who did not see a difference in the authority relations in the United States and the Home Country were included in the analysis. scientists state that the hierarchy of authority in the social system of work in the home country is based on seniority and age rather than accomplishments in the field. In the United States, seniority and age are seen by them as relatively unimportant in comparison to 172 the creativity and productivity of the scientists. As a consequence, the scientists feel that in the home country they are required to show deference to department chairmen, deans, administrators because they have been employed longer than they have or because of their senior age. Defference is expressed by maintaining a strictly formal relationship with the person in the authority position. In the United States, on the other hand, many of the people for whom they work are near the same age as the respondents and their relation— ships are quite informal. For example, one scientist from Germany said, "I call my 'boss' here, Bob, and we go out for a beer now and then. In Germany, I call my department chairman, Dr. '30 and so' and we restrict our talk to departmental matters.“ For these scien- tists, the social system of work in the home country resembles a formal gerontocracy. Scientists from developing nations freqfinmly cite an additional reason for the lack of collegiality in the home country. Most of their deans, department chair— men and administrators received their education many years ago and they have not kept up with the current developments in the field. The persons in authority positions maintain paradigms of their field which are outmoded. When the young faculty approaches them with a new idea, it is rejected outright because it does not fit into the conception of the field they possess. 173 Moreover, for many of these scientists, traditional cultural variables entered into these relationships with reference to age. As one Indian biologists said, "In my country, one has to pay homage to the aged, no matter if they are wrong or right." The quality of a scientists' project is not evaluated according to its scientific merits, but are viewed with suspicion because they came from a "youngster." These factors may account for the higher percentage of scientists from developing countries, as compared to scientists from developed coun- tries, who point out that the authority relations in the home country are less collegial than in the United States. Those scientists who viewed their interaction with persons in authority positions as being more collegial in the home country than in the United States usually make reference to the inaccessibility of contact with department chairmen, deans, and admini— strators. The only time they meet with these people are when they arrive and when they leave. Many felt that their leaders didn't even know of their presence. For example, one scientist related the following ex- perience: "I made an appointment to see the department chairman laazweek because I am leaving in three weeks. I wanted to thank him for giving me the opportunity to work here. When I went into his office, he said to me, I'd like to welcome you to our staff and I hope you 174 enjoy your stay with us." Moreover, what contact they have had with American's authority positions is limited to discussions of their work andznever goes beyond a strictly formal interaction. The important variable in the work situation for these scientists is the size of the department they are in as compared to the size of the staff in their home country. All of them believe that the presence of so many faculty members and students in the department in the united States prevent the emer- gence of meaningful formal and informal contacts with people in authority relations. In both cases, where scientists view their relations with persons in authority as being more collegial in the United States than in the home country or vise versa, an adjustment in the social identity of the scientists is likely to occur. In the former, the scientists must redefine their role as one of a colleague with the dep- artment chairmen, deans, etc. In the latter, the scien— tist must regard himself as one of the many members on a large staff. In short, these scientists are moving from one social system of work to another with reference to authority relations and must accommodate themselves to the differences in these systems. jDifferences Between Colleagues. In the preceding sec- tions, the differences between the social systems of work in the home country and the United States per- ceived by the respondents with reference to those below 175 them in rank (students) and those above them (dep- artment chairmen, deans, etc.) were analyzed. In this part, the analysis focuses on the variations scientists view as existing between scientists who are of an equivalent rank to them in the work situation in the home country and the United States, their col- leagues. On the interview schedule, the visiting foreign scholars were asked if they sawzxy differences in the work habits of their American counterparts as compared to their colleagues back home (question 55, interview schedule) and what was the nature of these differences (question 56, interview schedule). Fifty-three scien— tists saw such a difference, twenty-two did not, and seven did not respond. Those scientists who said their colleagues differed, outlined several dimensions of variations. Some regarded their American colleagues as working harder than their fel- low scientists in the home country. For example, one Australian scientist said, "My colleagues in Australia like to play the 'hoofies" (horses), but all these Americans can do is work hard. They don't know how to enjoy life." Others felt that their home country colleagues worked longer hours than American scientists because of the low level of research technology available in the home country. Some felt that Americans were not as dedicated to their work as their home country colleagues. Their 176 home country colleagues consider their work as central to their life, whereas for Americans work is, as one Japanese meteorologist related, "a nine to five job." Other respondents mentioned that Americans had less of an understanding of the problems confronting their field than their home country scientists because Americans were more interested in the technical aspects of a research project and less concerned with the theoretical and philosophical issues involved in a problem. Finally, some of the visiting foreigglscientists believed that Americans were more organized in their work than their home country colleagues due to the varying levels of technology and the financial support available for re- search in the United States as compared to the home country. On the basis of these responses, question 25 on the questionnaire was developed. Respondents were asked to determine the degree of their agreement or dis- agreement with the following statements: 1)"Americans in my field work harder than my colleagues back home“; 2)"My colleagues back home work longer hours than their American counterparts“: 3)"Americans are not as dedicated to their work as my home country colleagues“; 4)"The degree of understanding that Americans have of the problems con- fronting my field is less than that of my colleagues back home"; 5)“Americans are more organized in their work than my colleagues back home". The fequency distribution of 177 the type of agreement with these selected differences of American and home country colleagues is presented in Table 47. The majority agree that their American colleagues work harder and are more organized in their work than their home country colleagues. But they disagree with the statements that their home country colleagues work longer than Americans, that Americans are not as dedicated in their work as their home country counterparts, and that Americans have less of an understanding of the problems confronting their field than their home country scien- tists. Hence, it is only with reference to the work efforts of scientists and.organization in.their work ‘Untthese scientists regard their colleagues in the United States and the home country as differing. Systemic and role circumscription are related to agreement and disagreement on these items as Table 48 indicates. Scientists from developing nations agree that Americans work harder than their home country colleagues, that their home country colleagues work longer hours than their American counterparts and that Americans are more organized in their work than their country colleagues. Scientists from developed nations usually disagree with these statements. Scientists who performed teaching, administrative, or consulting roles also see Americans as working harder and being more organized in their work efforts than their home country colleagues. Those Ro.ooa m.m o.mm N.Nm as.am mmswmoaaoo maunsoo maom a: some xao: passe ca confismmao who: was mmmoHamsm mo.ooa m.m m.mH 0.50 RH.m mmswmmaaoo mapssoo meom a: smne caoam a: wsapsoam Isoo meoanoam mo wsaesmpmaou 1:: cm ac mmoa m>mm msmoaaoa< mmfi Ro.ooa Ro.oofi m.: m.: :.eH a.mm m.ea a.om Ro.m mm.mfi mmsmmmaaoo msmoaama< hapssoo msom sane mszom m< xaoz passe cs Lowsoq xao: doumoaemn mm poz mmzwmoaaoo ops mzmoanos< mapqsoo meom mmswmoafloo soozpmm monopommHQ mo came Aoafiuzv mmswmoaaoo mapqsoo oaom cam amoaaoa< mo moosopommfin empooaom and: unmamoaw< no 0959 mo scapsndaumaa owmpsmoaom Ro.ooa N.N m.mm H.~m No.0: mmsmmoaaoo zapnzoo msom zone smeamm ago: msmofiaoa< "as manta Hence mmzommmm Icoz UoUHomcss mmswmmfia omam< nonmemoLw¢ mo mama 179 respondents who perform research, teaching-research, and other low circumscribing roles, on the other hand, generally disagree with these statements, but believe that their home country colleagues work longer hours than Americans. Only tentative conclusions can be drawn con- cerning the degree of dedication in work and the under— standing of problems confronting a field of scientists in the United States and the home country because of the one-sided distribution of responses to these items. Scientists from developing nations and those scientists who perform high circumscribing roles, (teaching, admini- stration, or consulting) view Americans as more dedicated in their work and as having less of an understanding of problems confronting a field than their home country colleagues. Scientists from developed nations and those who perform low circumscribing roles disagree with these statements. The relationship of level of development (systemic circumscription) to the acknowledgement of differences between colleagues can be explained by the differences in the conditions of work in the countries where the educational and scientific development is low compared to high. In the developed nations, rewards are given for high levels of output in terms of completion of re- search projects and for publication of the results. In comparison, many of the developing nations such rewards omfi .sofipmmsu macs xsoz esp on momsoammsuso: no omsmoop scapaaaomesosfio oaaopmzm pom omOSp ems» aozoa mam msoapmamaaoo scapaasomesopao macs pom 2 one .mammamsm mega scam doggone mamas Sofia: momsommma 1:0: was momsoamoa emeaomcq: omsmoon oaa Hmsco pom mood soflpmampaoo some mom 2 annoy mn9* Ammuzv AooHuzv mmsmmoaaoo hangsoo mma. ems. meom a: sane ego: games as cowasmwao who: mam mumoaaos< Ammuzv Amoauzv mozwmmaaoo hapqsoo seem as saw. oom. mesa cause as msapzosczoo mamanoam mo madczmpmamesb so no mama ops: msmofisoe< Asmuzv AwOHuzv mmsmmmaaoo appssoo meow mm ems. 30:. ago: games 0» topmosetm mm no: mam mamoHpms< Aemuzv Asmuzv msmoauma< step mssom smmsoq mmm.1 mmfi. xao: mmswmoaaoo aspnsoo maom Ammuzv Amauzv mesmmoaaoo essence mom. mmm. msom 2mg» amcamm xaoz msmofiams¢ Amaom xaoz mo mahsv Apnoeaoam>oa go Ho>mqv mmswmmaaoo mapqsoo oeom cam soapaaaomasoaao oaom soapadaomesoafio caeopmmm smofipos< mo moosoaomman empomamm *mosmmmaaoo mppnzoo msom cum smoaaoe< mo moosopmumaa empomamm dam soapafisomssopao oaom cam anmpmzm nomzuom maofipmaoaaoo "m: manme 181 are absent. Secondly, in the United States, as in other similarly developed countries, there are more scientists competing for the same set of rewards, than in the developing nations. Hence, there are pressures placed on the scientist in developed nations to be pro- ductive which may be absent in the developing nations. These factors, account in part, for the reason why scien- tists from developing nations are more likely to see American scientists in their field as working harder than their home country scientists. Scientists from developing nations see their home country colleagues working longer hours and as less organized than Americans primarily because of the tech- nological and financial supports available for completing research tasks germane to a field. Without these forms of support, scientists from developing nations spend more time trying to complete the same jobs that scientists from developed nations are working on. In addition, the lack of technology and finances often make for a "patch-work" performance of work roles because the scientist pulls together from disparate sources support for his projects and spends time in maintaining any support he receives. Moreover, the lack of colleagues who can assist in the research endeavor critically affects the organization of the research enterprise. The scientists have relatively few people to whem they can turn for critiques and evaluations of their work. 182 As a result, many of the weaknesses and the strengths of the design of a research problem may be undetected. Such problems are less likely to occur in countries where there are such supports and colleagues. The relationship of type of work role performed in the home country (role circumscription) to the perception of differences between colleagues reflect the changes in the type of work role performed in the United States as compared to the home country. As will be shown shortly, those scientists who performed teaching, administrative, or consulting roles are more likely to experience a change in the type of roles they perform in the united States than scientists who have performed research, teaching-research, and.other low circumscribing roles. Scientists who are high- 1y“ role circumscribed may be comparing scientists in their home country who perform similar roles, i.e., teach- ing, administrative or consulting roles, to scientists in the United States who are performing low circumscribing roles, i.e., researchers, teaching-researchers, pro- fessional role players, and publication scientists. In so doing, the scientists are comparing across types of roles rather than within one form of role. So the dif- ferences they are viewing may be actually comparisons of types of roles and not general characteristics of American and home country scientists in their field. Unfortunately questions were not asked of these scientists to compare their colleagues in the United 183 States and their home country in terms of comparable roles. Some support of this argument can be given indirectly, however, if we examine the following data. On the interview schedule, the scientists were asked to identify those types of work roles which they regarded as being most central to their field in the home country and in the United States (questions 33 and 34). As tables 49 and 50 reveal, scientists identify the type of work role they perform in the home country to be the same as the type they regard as most central to their field in the home country{ That is, teachers, administrators, or consultants say their roles are most central to their field in the home country and those scientists who enact work roles such as basic and applied research, teaching-research, pro- fessional and publication roles in science see their roles astmost central in the home country. However, the former and the latter both agree that low circum- scribing roles (research, teaching-research, etc.) are most central to their field in the United States. This difference in the centrality of work roles in the United States as compared to the home country provides tentative support to my argument that scientists who perform teaching, 17'These results must be cautiously interpreted because of the high rate of non-responses to the questions. The discussion which follows, as a consequence, deals with trends. 184 administrative, or consulting roles may be comparing differences in colleagues on the basis of types of roles they perform rather than in terms of general character- istics of American and home country scientists in their field. Tables 49 and 50 also indicate that for scientists from developing nations teaching, administrative, or con- sulting roles are central to their field in the home coun- try. Scientists from developed nations, on the other hand, acknowledge research, teaching—research, organ- izational roles and activities in science, and scien- tific publication writing, as most central. Level of development of the home country is unrelated to type of work role centrality seen in the United States. Given these results on the centrality of work roles in the United States as compared to the home country and the differences of colleagues in these societies, one can conclude, that scientists from developing nations and those who perform teaching, administrative, or con- sulting roles are moving from one social system of work to another and that scientists from developed countries and those who perform low circumscribing roles may not necessarily be moving into a social system of work which is divergent from their home country work situation. Differences in the Type of Work Role Performed. In the prior sections of this chapter, the focus has been on the type of differences the visiting foreign scientists mmfi .maoa xaoz no sofipmmsv on momsommoalso: : go mmzmoop mm assoc no: mooc soflpmflaomasoaao macs pom 2 mass was. "a 50¢. "a Aanuzv Afimuzv Ammuzv Aomuzv wo.oofi so.oofi mo.oofi mo.ooa m.mm p.53 3.0: a.mm m.Hm m.sfi 0.3m m.mm mfi.mm mfi.mm mo.mm no.0: A.ouo sow» AwsfipHSmsoo nemaoam>omv 30a Amsaaoaobomv swam Imonanzm .noammm .soapmppmasase< Immuwsasomoa .msasoMoev swam .nohmmmmmv sou *soHpaHaomesoaHo maom sofipaaaomssoaao oasopmzm hapssoo oeom on» ad meHon .mpmfipsmaom map 0» Hmppsoo ma nods: oaom xao: mo mama use sodpaaaomesosfio maom cam oasmpmhm no: Hence omsoamomlsoz “.0po .soHpmoHanm scammmmmlwsasomma .soamomomv wsanaaom uazoaao maom sou Awsapazm usoo .sodpmspmasfiaem .wnfinomoav wsdnaaom uesosso oaom swam mapssoo meom a“ mamas on flamenco mHom xao: no mama oanma wwfi .mmaoa xaoz so soapmmSU one on momsoammalsos such mo mmsmoon mm Hmzvm no: mood scapaaaomesosao macs ace 2 one* gamuzv Afimuzv Ammuzv Aomuze ao.ooH so.oofi so.ooH mo.oos Hence «.mm m.as s.os s.mm mmsocmmmuzoz a.mm s.mm 0.0m m.mm m.opm .sodpsonflnsm scammmmmlwsanomme .soamommmv wzflnapom lemonao mHom son mo.sfi am.s at.m mm.mfi Awdapasm 1:00 .soapmnpmaqaad< .wsfisomopv maanaaom nanosfio oHom swam A.0po .soap Awsapazmsoo Admaoam>mmv 30g Amsaaoam>mov swam mopmom Bonds: 0:» utonansm .nosmmm .coHpmspmHnHeea ‘ as oaths op Immuwsflzomma .msasommav swam HmApsoo oaom .nosmommmv 30a xaoz mo maze #:OapafiaomESosHo mHom soHpQHLOmasoaHo ofismpmmm mmpmpm wands: 0:» ca mUHon .mpmapsmaom 0:» on Hmapsmo ma moan: mHom x903 wo make cam scapmaaomezoaao maom dam oHEopmmm "om canoe 187 seen as existing between the social systems of work in the home country and the United States. This part analyzes the transitions in the types or work roles performed in the United States as compared to the home country. Respondents were asked to identify the type of work role they performed in this country (question 17, questionnaire and 30 and 31, interview schedule). These work roles were classified according as either high role circumscribing or low role circumscribing. These two types were subsequently analyzed with reference to level of development (systemic circumscription) and type of work role performed in the home country (role circum- scription). Table 51 summarizes the results of this analysis. Almost all of the scientists are performing low circumscribing roles in the united States. Scientists from developing nations have a slightly greater tendency to perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles in the United States, than scientists from developed nations. unfortunately, a control table could not be constructed which would control for systemic circumscription (level of development) in order to establish whether scien— tists from developed or developing nations have greater transitions in the roles they play when they moved from the social system of work in the home country to that in the United States because of empty cells. However, since wma mmt. "a mam. no AamHuzv Afiwuzv Aflmfiuzv AHoHuzv Ro.ooH Ro.ooa Ro.ooa Ro.ooH Hmpoe m.m m.m 3.5 m.oH omsoammmlsoz 0.0m m.om 0.0m N.mu A.opo .oosmaom a“ :oHpmoHHnsm .noamomomlmsasomme .noammmomv :oHuaHaom nanosao mHom 304 sm.a mm.em me.o am.m Ammapasm usoo .soHpmppmHsHEc< .wsanommav soapaaaom nanotao oHom swam A.0po .soHp AwsfipHSmsoo Aomaon>on 30A Awsfiaoao>oov swam mmpmum ocean: esp twoaflnsm .zopmom soapmapmasa8d< ma mosaompmm Immuwsasomoa A.msH£omm9v swam maom xpoz no make a soamomomv 30A scapafipomasopao maom scuyadsomasoaao oasmpmmm mopmpm coeds: ms» ca omeaomamm oHom xaoz mo mama dam soaumasomssoaao oaom cam ofisopmhm "Hm magma 189 we have already shown that systemic circumscription is related to the types of work roles performed in the home country (role circumscription), we can tentatively con— clude that scientists from developing nations are more likely to experience a change in the type of work role they perform when they begin their work in the United It"; :- States than the scientists from developed nations. Table 52: Role Circumscription and Presence or Absence of Changes in the Type of Work Roles Performed in the United States as Compared to Those Performed in the Home Country Presence or Absence of Changes in Types of Work Roles Per- Role Circumscription formed in the United High Low (Research, States as Compared (Teaching, Teaching-Re- to Those Performed Administration, search, Publica— in the Home Country Consulting) tion, etc.) Change in Type of Work Role Per- formed 70.5% 7.3% No Change in Type of Work Role Per- formed 26.2 86.9 Non-Response 3.3 5.8 Total 100.0% 100.0% (N=61) (N=137) Q= .969 Type of work role performed in the home country is also positively related to the type of work role per— formed in the United States, that is, those scientists who perform high or low circumscribing roles in the home 190 country to some degree enact similar roles in the United States. This relationship which expresses a continuity in the roles played here and at home obscures the fact that scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles usually enact research, teaching- research, professional activities, and roles in science, or scientific publication writing in the United States. Over seventy per cent of these scientists experience a transition of roles whereas only seven per cent of those scientists who were researchers, teaching-re— searchers, scientific publication writing . and those who emphasized professional activities in science ex- perience such a change in roles. To indicate this absence or presence of changes in roles in the United States as compared to the home country, Table 52 was developed. .The relationship of role circumscription to transitions in the type of work roles performed in the united States clearly shows that scientists who perform high circumscribing roles in the home country generally experience a change in roles in the work situation in the United States and those who are low on role circumscription usually do not ex— perience such a transition. These results indicate, therefore, that scien- tists who are low on role circumscription are moving, not only between societies, when they enter the social system of work in the United States during their current 191 stay, but also they are being placed into roles which they have not been performing in the home country. Given the difference these scientists observe as ex— isting between students, and colleagues, the variations in their interactions patterns with individuals in authority positions, and the transitions they make in work roles , the experiences these scientists have in the social system of work are considerably divergent. from those they are likely to have in their home coun— tries. On the other hand, those scientists who are performing roles similar or identical to those WhiCh they have enacted in their home country will have ex- periences in this country comparable to those in their home country. Support or nonsupport for this.argument will rest on the results presented in the next section of this chapter. Differences in Work Involvement. A useful index of the changes which occur in the behavior of scientists as a result of participation in a social system of work that has characteristics which are divergent from the social system of work they have prior experience in are the changes in the degree of work involvement the scientists have in the United States as compared to the home country. The respondents were asked if they worked longer hours, if they worked less, and if they were more dedicated to their work in the United States or in their countries of origin (questions 22 192 questionnaire and 52, interview schedule). Affirmative responses to these questions can be regarded as a change in the behavior of the scientists and negative responses as no change in behavior in the work situation in the United States. Table 53: Percentage Distribution of Types of Ir? Differences in Work Involvement in the United States as Compared to the Home Country and the Acknowledgement of Differences in Work Involvement Acknowledge- a)Working b)Working c)More Dedication ment Dif— Longer Hours Less in the to Work in the ;fi ferences in in the U.S. U.S. than in U.S. than in " Work In- than in the the Home the Home volvement Home Country Country Country Yes 41.4% 9.0% 35.6% No 46.0 67.1 43.2 Non—Response 12.6 23.9 21.2 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=222) (N=222) (N=222) The frequency distribution of responses to these questions which appear in Table 53 indicated that the majority of scientists in this study did not ex- perience any changes in their work involvement during their stay in the United States. Generally, they said they were not wofidng longer hours, working less, or had no more dedication to their work in this country than in their home country. However, systemic and role circumscription both affect the changes in work involvement. 193 Table 54: Systemic and Role Circumscription and Differences in Work Involvement in the United States as Compared to the Home Country Systemic Low Circumscription Circumscription Low (Research, TeachingsRe— Differences High Low High (Teaching, search, W“* in Work (Devel- (Devel- Administration, Publication, Involvement oping) oped) Consulting) etc.) a)Working Longer hrs. , in the U.S. E than in the 1 Home Coun- is try " Yes 52.5% 30.6% 44.3% 38.0% No 29.7 58.6 41.0 51.8 None Response 17.8 12.8 14.7 10.2 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61) (N=137) Q= .544 Q= .192 b)Working less in the U.S. than in the Home Coun- try Yes 4.0% 13.2% 8.2% 9.5% No 62.4 70.2 65.6 71.5 Non- Response 33.6 16.6 26.2 19.0 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61) (N=137) Q: 0496 Q “0030 194 (Table 54: Systemic and Role Circumscription and (cont. Differences in Work Infolvement in the United States as Compared to the Home Country Systemic Low Circumscription Circumscription Low (Research, Teaching— Differences High Low High (Teaching, Research, in Work (Devel- (Devel— Administration, Publication, Involvement oping) oped) Consulting) etc.) c)More Dedication to Work in the U.S. than in the Home Country Yes 45.5% 27.3% 37.7% 34.3% No 30.7 52.9 34.4 49.6 Non- Response 23.8 19.8 27.9 16.1 Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% (N=101) (N=121) (N=61) (N=137) Q= .484 Q= .226 As Table 54 shows, scientists from developing nations and those who performed teaching, administrative, or consulting roles in the home country see themselves as working longer hours in the United States as com- pared to the time they spent working in their home country. In addition, they express a greater dedica— tion to work here than they did back home. Scientists from developed nations and those who enact research, teaching— research, and other low circumscribing roles do not ex- perience such transitions in work involvement. 195 The high degree of homogeneity of responses on the item "working less in the United States than in the home country“ prevents the formation of any con- clusions concerning the relationship of responses to the item and systemic and role circumscription. The N8 in the yes category cells are too small, for ex- ample, only one person from a developing nation said he was working less in the United States as com— pared to his home country. Moreover, the non-responses affect the direction of the relationship. As a conse- quence,there is no basis for interpretation of results on this item. On the basis of the other findings in this table, however, we can conclude that those scientists who are systemically circumscribed and role circumscribed will experience a change in the level of their work involvement in the United States. As has been suggested previously, such transitions occur because these scien- tists are in the process of redefining their role identi- ties in relationship to the students and colleagues they work with and to department chairmen, deans, and other persons in authority positions in the work situation in the United States. Since their interaction patterns with these groups and because their group attributes differ from their counterparts in the home country, these scien- tists increase their work efforts and dedication. Such a response is also due to the transitions in the types of 196 work roles they perform as a result of moving from one social system of work to another. Further Exploration in Differences Between Social Systems of Work. Exposure to dissociative experience may have important consequences for the acknowledgement of variations in the work situation in the United States as compared to the home country. To explore this possible relationship, correlations of the perception of differences between students and in relations with persons in authority positions to the varying types of dissociative experiences were computed and the results are presented in Table 55. In general, the acknowledgement of differences in relations with persons in authority positions and type of dissociative experience is unrelated. Only those scientists who have made trips to developing nations have a greater rate of seeing such a difference than scientists who have not made such journeys. Since it has already been shows that scientists from developing nations are more likely to make trips to ohter develop- ing nations than scientists from developed nations, the correlation of trips to developing nations and differen— ces in authority relations is supportive of the argu— ment made previously that scientists from developing na- tions are moving into a divergent social system of work in the united States than scientists from developed nations. 197 Except for this relationship, then, we can conclude that prior physical and psychic mobility has no effect on the perception of differences in the work situations in the United States and the home country with reference to authority relations. This finding implies that the sys- tem of authority relations in the United States is unique to this country and that prior experiences in other coun- tries or with their members does not necessarily prepare the respondents for the type of experiences with persons in authority positions they encounter in the united States. The perception of differences between students in the United States and the home country, in comparison, is related to types of dissociative experiences but in varying directions. Those scientists who have had fo- reign educational experiences, contact with foreigners in the home country, belong to foreign scientific asso- ciations, or who have made journeys to developed coun- tries usually see no difference between students here and at home. The respondents who have worked abroad be- fore or who have taken trips to developing countries, on the other hand, generally say that students in the United States are different than their counterparts in the home country. This finding indicates that certain forms of prior physical and psychic mobility acquaint scientists with a range of divergent patterns of student- professor relationships and varying characteristics of students in other countries similar to those which exist Table 55: Types of Dissocia- tive Ex- periences Foreign Ph.D. Educational Experience Foreign Work Experience Trips to De— veloped Coun- tries Trips to De- veloping Countries Interaction with Foreign- ers Memberships in Foreign Scientific Associations 198 Correlations of Differences Between Social Systems of Work in the united States and the Home Country and Type of Dissociative Experience: Differen- ces Between Students and in Relations with Persons in Authority Positions Differences Between Social Systems of Work in the united States and the Home Country: Differences Differences in Between Authority Student§ Relations (N=186) (N=200)* -0365 .126 .199 -.041 -.277 -.041 .263 .159 -.173 -.O78 -0298 -.017 * The Ns do not equal 222 because of non-responses to the questions dealing with differences between students and authority relations. in the united States. Foreign work experience and making trips to developing countries does not seem to yield such information. Previously, it has been shown that systemic and role circumscription are related to dissociative experience, that is, scientists from developing countries and those 199 who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles in the home country are more likely to be phy- sically and psychically mobile than scientists from developed countries and those who enact research, teaching-research, professional, or publication roles. In this chapter, we have also seen that these scientists usually acknowledge a difference between students in the home country and the united States. .These findings and the results presented above indicate that prior educa— tional experience abroad, interaction with foreigners in the home country, memberships in foreign scientific associations, and trips to developed countries by scientists from developing countries and those who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles prepare these scientists for the type of experiences they encounter in the social system of work with reference to students. Accordingly, these scientists have less of an adjustment to make to the work situation in the united States than scien— tists who have not had these forms of prior psychic and physical mobility. Such an occurrence was related in the interviews by a scientist from India who had been to Great Britain for his doctorate and who had made several trips to the Uni- ted States, France, and England. On a probe to the ques- tion on differences between students, he said the follow- ing:4Ewmy time I've been abroad I have met students in the countries I visited. Qverall, they are the same in 200 every country. They treat their professors in a simi- lar manner and are very similar in the way they work in the laboratory. Indian students do the same work as British, French, and American students in my field. There is no difference between them. I don't have any problems at all with students." Those scientists who have had some forms of dis- sociative experience, then, are less likely to see a difference between students in the home country as compared to American students than scientists who have not had these experiences. Prior experience in foreign countries and with their members, on the other hand, have no effect on differences scientists see in their relations with persons in authority positions. In summary, this chapter has shown that many scien— tiSts regard the work situation in the united States as being considerably different than the work milieu in the home country. It differs in terms of the types of rela— tionships they establish with students, colleagues and those persons who occupy authority positions in the social system of work. The work situation also varies with reference to the work habits and dedication of the people they encounter in these settings. We have also seen that the work roles they perform in one sys- tem may not necessarily be the same they perform in another. In short, for many of the respondents the current stay in the united States is a movement from one social system of work to Another which is unlike 201 the social system of origin. They are not moving into an identical system. On this basis, we can conclude that for many of the respondents the current stay in the united States is a dissociative experience. Scientists from developing nations and those scientists who perform administrative, teaching, or consulting roles usually view the work situation in the united States as divergent from the social system of work in the home country, experience a change in the type of work roles they perform in the united States, and increase their involvement in work in the united States. For scientists from developed countries and those who perform research, teaching—research, profes- sional, and publication roles, on the other hand, the work situation in the united States, the type of work roles they enact, and the degree of their work involve- ment are, in general, comparable to the home country. Therefore, the following proposition holds for this group of visiting foreign scientists: The greater the systemic and role circumscription, the greater the difference of the social system of work in the united States as compared to the home country. This proposi- tion implies that the social system of work in the United States is more likely to be dissociative for scientists from developing countries and those who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles 202 in the home country than for scientists who are from developed nations and those who enact research, teach— ing-research, publication, or professional roles in the home country. Chapter 6 Summary of Results and Conclusions 203 204 This study attempted to locate variations in the behavior and attitudes of a sample of visiting foreign scientists from developed and developing countries. In addition, several variables were suggested as some of the sources of these divergent interaction patterns and orientations. Three major conclusions can be drawn from the results presented in preceding chapters: 1) systemic circumscription (level of development of the educational and scientific institutions of the home country) and role circumscription (type of work role performed in the home country) have an impact on the social relationships, prior experience, and current behavior and perspectives of the visiting foreign scientists; 2) exposure to dissociative experiences (movement from one social system to another social system and exposure to the ideas, technology, or cultu- ral values of a ”foreign" social system or systems through contact with the actors of another system) fos- ters post—modernity, worldmindedness, and a universa- listic orientation to social interaction, and, to some degree, work location, but its effect on third cultural network involvement and a universaliStic orientation to living location is mixed; and 3) the remarkable degree of uniformity in the behavior and attitudes of these scientists gives some evidence of a trans-societal and cultural scientific community. 205 Systemic and Role Circumscription. Systemic and role circumscription, as evidenced in the level of development of the home country and the type of work roles performed respectively, are related to each other and to exposure to dissociative experien— ces, societal social responsibility, professional participation, perspectives on a stratification of nations in scientific fields, and the differences between the social systems of work in the united States as compared to the home countries of the respondents. Their relationship to scientific social responsibility and non-professional responsibility are, however, either inconclusive or unrelated. The relationship of systemic circumscription to role circumscription implies that scientists from de- veloping nations are more likely to perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles than their collea- gues in the developed world. In those countries with a low level of development of educational and scienti— fic institutions, there is inadequate support given for those roles which are most central to the creation and utilization of scientific knowledge. Basic and even applied research often take a back seat to teaching, administration, or consulting. As Dedijer has pointed out (1962) and as these results confirm, many scientists are from segments of “pre-research"cultures or societies. 206 Given the conditions of the home country com- bined with the lack of availability of research and professional roles in science, and the relatively little support given to research productivity and publication and the utilization of these results, visiting foreign scientists from developing nations who are highly role circumscribed have experiences in other societies and with their members. That is, the greater the role circumscription, the greater the exposure to dissociative experiences. They have been abroad for educational reasons, have made jour- neys to other nations, interact with foreigners in the home country, and belong to scientific associa- tions in nations other than their home country orga- nizations. Through such activities, they keep up with the current developments in their field, make them— selves visible to scientists in other nations, and establish collegial relationships with them. Their performance of teaching, administrative or consulting roles, however, hinders their chances of employment in other nations as the negative correlation of the performance of these types of highly circumscribing roles to cross-societal work experiences, controlling for systemic circumscription,has shown. Similarly, scientists from developed nations who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles make trips to developed countries, interact with foreigners and belong to scientific associations in 207 other nations. But their colleagues in developed na- tions who enact research, teaching—research, profes- sional, or publication roles have been abroad for their doctorate and have made trips to developing nations. Systemic and role circumscription are also related to social responsibility at a societal level. Scientists from developing nations and scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles are concerned with the possible implications of their work on their society and others. In addi- tion, they often use the current problems facing their home country as a criterion for the selection of a research problem were feasible. Moreover, they give approval to the involvement of scientists in national decision-making. Incontradistinction, scientists from developed nations and those who perform research, teaching— research, professional, or publication roles in their home countries are commonly low on societal social responsibility. Concerning the second level of social responsibility, the scientific, the results are inconclusive. Most of the scientists in the study express an obligation to the next generation of scientists. They also choose problems rele- vant to science for research topics. Because of the low rate of disagreement to these indices of scientific social responsibility, a generalization concerning their relationship to systemic and role circumscription cannot 208 be made. The results show, however, that the type of obligation to the next generation is correlated in a positive direction to systemic circumscription. That is, scientists from developing nations are com— mitted to the growth of science in their own nations, whereas scientists from developed nations state that their obligation to the next generation is in refer- ence to the international scientific community. The scientific social responsibility of scientists, then, is particularistic for scientists from developing nations, but universalistic for scientists from deve- loped nations. This obligation by scientists from developing countries to build the national scientific community in their field rather than the international scienti- fic community is one way of achieving mobility in the stratification system of science described by the re- spondents. Scientists from developed nations and those who perform teaching-research, research, professional, or publishing roles in their home countries see their nations as centers or leaders in their fields. Scien- tists from developing countries and those who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles identify the positions of their home country in a ranking of nations as low or peripheral to the centers of research in their fields. The peripheral or center rank of the respondents' home countries affects the type of positions they occupy when they are employed in one of the centers 209 in their fields, the united States, and the type of exchanges they see existing between their home countries and the united States. As a consequence, scientists from the periphery do not receive the same level of support as do scientists from the center in the united States and in the systemic lin- kages between their nations and the united States. To compete as equals with the centers, those scien— tists from the periphery must support the growth of viable research institutes in their countries. Thus scientists from developing countries are committed to the next generation of scientists in their country and not throughout the world. A strengthening of science internationally desired by scientists from developed nations may be considered as a form of neo-colonialism. Because those outside of the centers of science in their fields have relatively little chance to improve their position, the growth of viable research institutes in the peripheral nations will provide more scientists for the centers and thus increase the dominance of the centers over the periphery. Furthermore, those scientists from the periphery,i.e., the developing nations, are more likely to hold non-main- line positions (research assistant and associate) than mainline positions (instructor to full professor) in the United States. The opposite is true for scientists from the center, i.e., developed nations. Moreover, scientists 210 from developing nations are more likely to consider the systemic linkage networks of exchanges of students, resources, and information between their home countries and one of the centers in then? fields, the united States, as being non—reciprocal than scientists from developed nations whoview these networks as reciprocal. The stress placed on problems facing the home country as a criterion for a choice of research pro- blems and the emphasis on training future generations to create and further the national scientific institu- tions by systemically and/or role circumscribed scien- tists would lead one to expect that this societal and social responsibility would foster participation in extra— scientific affairs and activities. It does not.Scien— tists from developing nations or those who perform teaching, administrative, orconsulting roles do not participate in civic, political, religious, etc. groups more than scientists from developed nations and those who perform research, teaching-research, professional, or publication roles. The lack of participation of scientists in these organizations and activities and in any form of social action which would lead to change in their society implies that these visiting foreign scientists restrict most of their activities to the scientific community. Any involvement in social change and in activities outside of science by these scientists, if it occurs at all, is possible only as a direct or in- direct outcome of the research they are engaged in or 211 some activity within the social system of science. This group of visiting foreign scientists in the United States are, in short, one system actors. Systemic and role circumscription are, however, correlated in a reverse direction to participation in scientific activities as proposed in Proposition 11. Scientists from developed nations and those who are teachers, administrators, or consultants have higher rates of attendance at national scientific meetings and of paper and article publication than scientists from developing nations and those who enact research, teaching-research, professional, or publication roles. Of the few scientists who have published books, most are teachers, administrators, or consultants. These results support the analysis of the impact of systemic and role circumscription on the behavior of scientists in the first chapter. In developing nations, little support is given to research output in terms of inputs from society to science, yet such an output is demanded. Moreover, most of the avail— able roles in science that scientists from these nations can perform are those which do not lead to research productivity in terms of scientific publications. In addition, the positive correlations of systemic and role circumscription to memberships in scientific asso- ciatons in other nations and their negative correlation 212 to attendance at national scientific meetings implies that scientists from developing nations consider scien— tific associations in other countries as more important to their career than the associations in their home country in terms of accessibility to information concerning current developments in their fields and to meet colleagues. Without viable or highly developed scientific associations in their home country these scientists use memberships in associations in other nations as a systemic linkage to the larger scientific community. Scientists from developing nation and those scientists who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles view the work situation in the united States as different from the social system of work in their home countries. The social systems of work varies in the interaction patterns these scientists have with students, col- leagues, and persons in authority positions (e.g., department chairmen, deans, supervisors) and in terms of the characteristics students and colleagues in their home countries and the united States. In addition, these scientists experience a change in the type of work roles they perform in the united States as compared to those they enacted in the home country. In this country, they usually perform research, teaching-research, professional, or publication roles. In their countries of origin they generally enact teaching, administrative or consulting roles. Moreover, in response to the differences in the 213 social systems of work, these scientists increase their work involvement in the united States and, as has been suggested, must redefine their roles and social iden— titles with their American colleagues, students, and supervisors. For scientists from developed nations and those who perform research, teaching-research, professional, or publication roles, the work situation in the united States is quite similar to the social system of work in their home countries in terms of students, colleagues, persons in authority positions and relation with them, the type of work roles they enact here, as compared to their home country, and the degree of their work involvement. On the basis of these findings, the fol- lowing propostion is formulated: the greater the systemic and role circumscription, the greater the difference of the social system of work in the united States as com- pared to the home country. Hence, the social system of work in the united States is now likely to be dissociative for scientists from developing countries and those who perform teaching, administrative, or consulting roles in their home country than for scientists who are from developed nations and those who enact research, teaching— research, publication, or pfoessional roles in the home country. In conclusion, the level of development of the educational and scientific institutions in the home 214 country and the type of role performed by scientists are important variables determining the possibilities of exposure to dissociative experiences, social respon— sibility at the societal level, the type of obligation to future generations of scientists, and experiences in the social system of work in the united States. Dissociative Experiences. Exposure to dissociative experiences, i.e., foreign educational and work ex- perience and trips to developed and developing coun- tries (physical mobility) and interaction with foreign- ers in the home country and memberships in foreign scientific associations (psychic mobility), is also a significant source for explaining variations in the perspectives and orientations of the scientists in this study. Psychic mobility and physical mobility contributes to the growth of a post-modern perspective on the future of the home country, fosters interna- tionality, and a universalistic orientation to social interaction, and to work. The relationship of dissociative experience to third cultural network involvement is, however, mixed. Only two of the four physical mobility indices, trips to developed and developing nations, are strongly re- lated to such involvement. The data suggests that these journeys given an experiential base for in- teraction with members from other societies that 215 educational and work experience do not give. This explanation is feasible in one considers the purpose of the trips to these nations and the nature of the educational and work experiences and the status- roles of the persons making these trips. The scien- tists who made trips to other countries did so as in peers for the purpose of directly contacting scientists from other nations or to become acquaiuied with a socio-cultural milieu different from their own. Either purpose implies co—equal participation of the respon— dents with selective members from another society. Educational and work experiences abroad would seem to involve such participaton. However getting a higher degree in a foreign insititution or working in a foreign nation is seemingly quite restricting. WOrking towards a degree or being employed for a certain period of time may require a total commit- ment to these tasks obliterating the chance to participate in social and professional relation- ships with members of the host and other societies and to absorb social and cultural patterns of the pro- fession in the host society. Therefore, when these scientists went abroad for advanced educational and work purposes, their most significant and enduring contacts with people from societies other than their own are limited to the classroom or the laboratory or the equivalent and does not extend outside of 216 these settings. During these experiences, the nature of interaction is restricted to scientific matters and does not run over into social areas. This is especially true if there are linguistic problems. The two psychic mobility indices are also ambi- valently related to third cultural network participa- 377 tion. Interacting with foreigners in the home country is functional for using scientific meetings and con— ferences as a setting for persmal social relation— ships with people from other societies, but not for establishing networks at work or outside of work in the united States. Belonging to associations in other nations, on the other hand, does lead to third cultural social relationships outside of the work situation with people who work at the same place of work and for interaction with people from other nations at scientific meetings. It leads to non-third cultural relationships outside of work with people who work 18 elsewhere or who are in the same field as the respondent. Given these results, proposition 3, the greater the exposure to dissociative experiences, the greater the involvement in third cultural networks, can be accepted only with reservations. That is, only in specific situations and only with some types of dis— sociative experiences does third cultural network in- volvement follow from exposure to dissociative eXperienceS. 18 Interaction with members from the home country in a dif- ferent society acting only with each other can be part of a third culture insofar as they are mediating the two cultures and are sharing norms which transcend both societies. 217 Exposure to dissociative experiences is also related to a particularistic orientation to living location, i.e., scientists are selective in choosing residential site. Only trips to other nations apart from educational and work experiences has led to such a universalistic orientation, i.e., no preference for a living location. This finding suggests an evaluative ability on the part of those actors who have had these experiences. By becoming more fully aware of alterna- tives to their society and culture in their status as professional scholar, the majority make a preference for their home country or another country. Exposure to dissociative experiences, in summary, has had an effect on the way these scientists view the world and their relationship to it, their per- spective on the future of their home country, and their orientation to where they would like to live or work and who they would interact with. In general, this effect has been in the direction of increasing the post-modernity, worldmindedness, and universalistic orientation of the visiting foreign scientists. Homogeneity in Science. In Chapter 1, the main point of emphasis was an exploration of the variations in the behavior and attitudes of the scientists and the sources of this heterogeneity. The variations that have been found to exist on the selected behavioral — 218 and attitudinal dimensions has been related to systemiC' and role circumscription and exposure to dissociative experiences. Yet, there is one finding as consistent as the others: the majority shared the same beliefs, perspectives, and orientations and tended to behave in a similar fashion both in and outside of the scien- tific community. These visiting foreign scientists have, for the '- "u ILA-1‘21: J”. \ _ ’ é I most part, experiences in other societies or with : people from them. They are involved in thrid cultural .1 I!" m A ’o .. networks and their world views have changed in the direction of increasing worldmindedness as a result of these contacts. Moreover, they visualize the future of their home country in dynamic post-modern terms wherein their nation acts in cooperation with other nations to shape the future of the world. They believe that they are socially responsible to their society and the world and to the international and their nation— al scientific communities. In addition, they use the same criteria for selecting a location for work, yet they are universalistically oriented to a locus of work. They share a particularistic orientation to a living location, but express a universalism with regard to whom they are willing to associate with. They participate readily in their national scientific associations, and have similar publication rates. Finally, they tend to be non—participants in extra-scientific organizations and activities. l! 219 This homogeneity of responses gives credence to the idea that there is an international scientific community, at least with regards to these scientists. Such a uniformity in behavior and attitudes could only emerge out of a common social and normative structure. The main thesis of this study, therefore, must be re— vised. Science is not a heterogeneous collectivity. It is a homogeneous collectivity characterized by varying levels of systemic and role circumscription, in which scientists share the same beliefs, per- spective, and life styles, the world, their socie— ties, and their place in them. The scientific com- munity, therefore transcends societal and cultural differences resulting in, what one may call, a third culture of science. Appendix A Interview Schedule and Questionnaire 220 .wvh "J. 4} fiifilimn 7‘ y , y A STUDY IN THE socromsr OF SCIENCE INTERVIEW SCHEDULE 221 T" Sal P. Restivo C.K. VanderPool Dept. of Sociology Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan 222 Page one FACE SHEET 1. Sex: M F 2. Age 3. Marital Status: S M D W 4. Number of children: 5. Birthplace: 6. Citizenship: 7. Current Position: Visiting Nonvisiting Instructor Professor Ass't. Professor Research Assoc. Assoc. Professor Other (specify) 8. Field: 9. Educational Background: Country Field Degree& Year A. UNDERGRADUATE: B. GRADUATE: C. POST-GRADUATE: Page one o‘. .4 c..." o - n ,- . , - o , - I a", w...- .a .- _—fi. 0. . ._ .~ ‘- ». » ..- — , . . (Lo 3.” i ‘— ~—.—~»—- - -v-‘u - 1.... 0‘1 -o- \w 10. 11. 12. 13. 1223 Career History for LAST FIVE YEARS: A. Organization: Industry Government University Other (specify) Page two 8. Countries: C. Positions: IF MARRIED: Background of Spouse: A. CITIZENSHIP: B. BIRTHPLACE: r”! . .— r1" 0. 22911211121: Background of Mother: A. CITIZENSHIP: B. BIRTHPLACE: C. OCCUPATION: Background of Father: A. gunners: B. BIRTHPLACE: c. scam: Page two . ~Io— .. ‘ - __.- ~ "“4 ‘ . -. a as» ..-’ .- ‘ -. ‘ _..,.. .Ih - v a 0 ...-- .u. " t n .14.’ .-~ . 1 ‘. ...- s 224 Page three 14. Not counting this trip, what foreign countries have you visited in the last five years for reasons related to your work? COUNTRY LENGTH OF STAY -3 \_ "L .4 ‘ .c’ “.7! 15. IF MARRIED: Is your wife with you? YES NO 1 u‘m‘l_ 16. IF MARRIED: Are your children with you? YES NO 17. Do you interact regularly with foreigners in your home country? YES NO IF NO, GO TO 21 18. WHAT COUNTRIES WERE THEY FROM? 19. WHAT WERE THEIR OCCUPATIONS? 20. WHY DID YOU INTERACT WITH THEM? Page three 22. 23. 24. 2255 Page four 21. .IF RESPONDENT HA3 CONTACTED SCIENTISTS; WHAT WERE YOUR OBLIGATIONSHAND”RESPONSIBILITIES TO THEM? How did you happen to come to this country? A. To do research with colleagues 8. To learn about new technioues C. To see America D. To communicate results with colleagues E. To do research which could not be done at home F. To teach C. Other (specify) \\ \ \\\ How did you happen to come to this university? A. I was invited by colleagues B. Invitation by university C. University provided funds to do what I want to do D. A friend of mine was here before E. Exchange program F. Other (specify) \H H What do you plan to do after your stay here? 25. WHY? Page four ”WP K I-- Ai\--. . 226 Page five C TEACHING WORKTROLE IF THE RESPONDENT HAS TAUGHT BEFORE AND IS CURRENTLY TEACHING, ADMINISTER THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS 1. What level courses are you teaching? 2. What are the differences you have experienced in teaching here as compared to teaching in your home country? 3. What do you like most about teaching here? 4. What do you like least? 5. In your field, when does a man reach his peak as a teacher? 6. What impact does teaching have on your professional career, e.g. is it detrimental to your status in the field, etc.? 7. DOES THIS VARY IF YOU TEA H IN YOUR HOME COUNTRY AS COMPARED TO TEACHING THE UNITED STATES? IF YES: 8. IN WHAT WAYS: 9. Is there a difference in the way you teach (field) here as compared to your home country? (Probe for materials added to a course which are not part of the general subject matter of the course.) ll. 12. 227 Page five B WORK ROLE UNITED STATES Teaching formal courses and seminars (Including preparation time) Basic research Applied research Hll Research and develOpment Teaching-Research (that kind of research carried on with one or more apprentice researchers for whom this research in- volvement is part of their formal training) I | Administration within an organization Public service activities (speeches to general public, appearances on T.V. and radio, papularization of science representing your field at civil functions, etc.) Consultant to public organisations IH Consultant to private organizations Organizational activities in science (editing, membership participation, committee participation in scientific organizations, etc.) Writing and publication 1 Other (specify) Page five B. ., ~. . 10. ll. 12. 228 WORK ROLE HOME COUNTRY Teaching formal courses and seminars (Including preparation time) Basic research Applied research Research and deveIOpment Teaching-Research (that kind of research carried on with one or more apprentice researchers for whom this research in- volvement is part of their formal train- ing. Administration within an organization Public service activities (apeaches to general public, appearances on T.V. and radio, populariaation of science, representing your field at civil fun- ctions, etc.) Consultant to public organizations Consultant to private organizations Organizational activities in science (editing, membership participation, committee participation in scientific organisations etc.) Writing and publication Other (specify) Page five A. Page five A 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 229 Page five Who provided the financial support for your trip here at this time? Home country U.S. Other A. Government: B. Industry: C. Foundation: D. University: E. U.N. Agencies: F. Personal resources: G. Other (specify): Who supports your work here? WORK ROLE On this sheet are a list of activities scientists sometimes perform. CHECK THOSE ACTIVITIES YOU PERFORM BACK HOME. (GET RANK ORDER) WHICH ACTIVITIES TOOK UP MOST OF YOUR TIME? (GET RANK.ORDER) Here is a sheet identical to the one I just gave you. CHECK THOSE ACTIVITIES WHICH YOU PERFORM HERE. (GET RANK ORDER) WHICH ACTIVITIES TAKE UP MOST OF YOUR TIME HERE? (GET RANK ORDER) Page five T‘- -s<-~—It m E. nurses—mm I v...-.-_. *~o-~— .. w.. . . I _ ‘ .I . < 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 23C) Page six Which of the above activites, do you regard as most central to your role as a FIELD? Which of the above activities are most central to FIELD in your home country? Which of the above activities are most central to FIELD in the United states? What types of activities will you perform when you go back home? A. The same as before 8. The same as here C. Different (specify As a result of this trip to the United States, do you plan to change the way you perform your activities? YES NO Ig:NO. GO TO 39. 37. IN WHAT WAYS WILL YOU CHANGE YOUR ACTIVITIES? Page six o ‘ ,,,,,_ .w, 231 Page seven 38. DO YOU PLAN TO CONTINUE TO WORK IN THE SAME FIELD? 39. What are the networks of exchanges between American and H,C, Field? A. Jobs B. Money C. Resources D. Journals E Q! E. Equipment F. Students lllllll G. News and gossip H. Work contacts with other scientists QROBE FOR DIRECTION OF EXCHANGES) 40. WHAT IS YOUR ROLE IN THESE NETWORKS? (PROBE FOR OBLIGATIONS g: PROFESSIONAL COMMUNITY) 41. Have you interacted with students in your home country? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 44 Page seven -- «‘e ‘ " fl-upo- 1 ”234. Page eight 42. DO YOU FIND THAT THERE Is A DIFFERENCE IN THE WAY YOU INTERACT WITH STUDENTS HERE As COMPARED TO YOUR INTERACTION WITH STUDENTS BACK HOME? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 44 43. WHAT IS THE NATURE OF THE DIFFERENCE? hes-I I 4 44. Do you find that there is a difference in the way you interact with persons in authority positions here (6.3. Dept. chairman, Deans, etc.) as compared with the way you interact with similar individuals back home? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 46 45. WHAT IS THE NATURE OF THE DIFFERENCE? 46. In your current stay here who are the nationals you most frequently interact with? 47. WHAT DO YOU USUALLY TALK ABOUT? a. work (research) b. social matters c. other (Specify) 48. Did you know these scientists personally before you came to this country? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 51 49. HOW DID YOU GET TO KNOW THEM? 50. HOW WELL DO YOU KNOW THEM? Page eight 51. 52. 53. 55. 57. 58. 59. 60. 235 Page nine Some individuals are completely involved in their research --- absorbed by it night and day. For others their work is simply one of several interests. IN YOUR HOME COUNTRY, HOW INVOLVED WERE YOU IN RESEARCH? Completely involved Somewhat involved Not much invloved Has the level of your work involvement changed since you've been here? Is there anything about your work experiences here which you did not expect before coming here? YES‘ NO IF NO: GO TO 55 _ '1. 11 d‘ 1!.‘4 x 54. WHAT DIDN'T YOU EXPECT? Are the work habits of your American colleagues different from the work habits of your colleagues back home? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 57 56. IN WHAT WAYS ARE THEY DIFFERENT? What do you like most about working with Americans? What do you like least about working with Americans? COMMITTMENT TO SOCIAL CHANGE Are you involved in bringing about change in your country? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 65 What type of change are you working for? Page nine 61. 62. 63. £236 Why are you involved? a. It is my duty to my country b. Every scientist must c. To better mankind d. My work role necessitates it e. It's an Opportunity for advancement f. Other (Specify) Page ten What is the nature of your involvement? 8. Membership in political groups b. Applied research c. Planning groups d. Basic research e. Teaching f. Consulting 3. Kinship and friendship groups b. Other (specify) IHIHII Are there any constraints placed on your involve- ment? YES 64. WHAT ARE THESE CONSTRAINTS AND WHO IMPOSES THEM? A. Constraints: a. Social values cannot be investigated b. Cannot attack groups in power c. Cannot criticise ideology d. Cannot invistigate cer- tain physical problems e. Illegal to pursue these activities f. Other (specify) NO IF NO: GO TO 66 Page ten 65. 66. 67. 237 Page eleven B. Imposers: a. b. Co d. 3. GO TO 66 Public in general Government and other authorities Religious groups Traditional leaders Other (specify) Why aren't you involved? a. Outside the role of a scientist b. Too involved in my work to bother with it c. Fear the reprisals of such invol- vement d. Cannot because others prevent me HH e. Other (specify) IF RESPONDENT ANSWERS WITH STATEMENTS SIMILAR TO c&d, PROBE THE CONSTRAINTS AND THE IMPOSERS OF CONSTRAINTS. How do you feel about scientists who are directly involved in bringing about change in your society? IMAGES OF THE FUTURE If you could picture your country in the best possible form, how would things look about ten Years from now? Page eleven .memnvr5'x. ". —— 39"— ' H‘_—._ ‘2“ 68. 69. 70. 71. 74. 238% Page twelve What models do you use to construct this ideal picture of your country? What segments of your society share such models? Are the scientists in your country important for achieving this future? Do you think this ideal future will be achieved by your country? YES NO 72. IF NO: WHY NOT? 73. IF YES: WHY? Are you pessimistic or Optimistic about the future of the world? PESSIMISTIC OPTIMISTIC 75. WHAT IS THE BASIS OF YOUR PESSIMISM OR OPTIMISM? Page twelve via; ,. Ir£!~'a . 76. 80. 239 Page thirteen SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY Some scientists are concerned about the effects of their work on society (e.g., the atomic scientists who expressed concern about the use of atomic wea- pons, biologists who are concerned about indiscrim- inate crOp Spraying). Other scientists are not concerned (e.g., mathematicians working on theoret~ ical problems which have no applied sapects). Are you concerned about the effects of your work on society? YES In what way? No Why not? IF YES,GO TO PROBE 77. IF NO, GO TO 78. 78. Are you a member of any associations or group of Scientists interested in the effects of scientific research on society? YES (names of organizations) NO IF NO, GO TO 79. 79. Would you be interested in joining such an organization? WHY OR WHY NOT? KINSHIP How have your family and relatives influenced your professional career? Page thirteen 81. 82. 83. 85. 86. 240 Page fourteen ORGANIZATIONAL PARTICIPATION What scientific organizationcb you belong to? Is there a viable scientific association in your field in your home country? What are the goals of the scientific organizations you belong to? 84. WHAT ARE THEIR SOCIETAL FUNCTIONS? How often do you attend meetings at the very often often seldom never a. Regional level . b. National level c. International level What meetings have you attended in the U.S.? Page fourteen 89. 90. 91. 93. 241 Page fifteen 87. WHY DO YOU ATTEND THESE MEETINGS? a. To meet old friends b. To establish new relations c. To advance my career d. To meet with other peOple doing the same research e. To advance the career of students f. To keep up with develOpments of the field 3. Other (specify) 88. WHAT LANGUAGES ARE USED AT THE INTERNATIONAL MEETINGS YOU HAVE ATTENDED? How would your colleagues describe your role in the profession? How many papers have you published in journals? l-lO ll-20 Over 20 None Where do you usually publish? 92. WHY DO YOU PUBLISH IN THESE PLACES? In what languages do you write for publication? Page fifteen dn-mmem )- .'. _' A. 96. 97. 98. 99. 242 Page sixteen 94. HAS YOUR WORK BEEN TRANSLATED INTO OTHER LANG- UAGES? 95. DO YOU TRANSLATE PUBLISHED WORKS? Who reads what you publish? To whom would you send preprints and reprints of your articles? PROBE FOR COUNTRY. How many books have you published? 1-5 6-10 Over 10 None Have you published papers and books in subjects other than your own? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 101. 100. IN WHAT AREAS? Page sixteen 101. 243 Page seventeen THIRD CULTURAL RELATIONS Have scientific gatherings in nations other than your home country enabled you to establish personal and communication ties wiht scientists from other nations? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 108. 102. WHERE WERE THEY HELD? 103. WHAT LANGUAGES WERE SPOKEN? 104. WHAT NATIONS DID THE SCIENTISTS COME FROM? 105. HOW DO YOU KEEP IN TOUCH? a. Letters b. Visits c. Other meetings d. Other (Specify) 106. WHAT DO YOU USUALLY TALK ABOUT? 107. HOW WELL DO YOU KNOW THEM? GO TO 109. Page seventeen 241+ Page eighteen 108. How do you establish such relationships? 109. Do you ever visit a country specifically to meet with scientists in your field? . d b I a -. . . is I . Ir.‘ a! E :5 110. When visiting another country, do you look up scien- tists? YES______ NO____ ENQQQIQIIZ' 111. WHY? 112. Have any of the Scientists you met abroad visited you here in the U.S.? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 116. 113. FHY? 114. WHAT WERE YOUR OBLIGATIONS AND RESPONSIBILITIES TO THEM? 115. WHICH COUNTRIES WERE THEY FROM? Page eighteen 116. 117. 118. 122. 245 Page nineteen In your relations with foreign scientists, is your national identity a liability or an asset? 8. Liability b. Asset c. Both d. Doesn't make a difference e. Don't know Pi in. (b GO TO 124. WHY? DOES THIS PROBLEM ARISE WITH SCIENTISTS FROM CERTAIN NATIONS? YES NO LE N0: GO TO 122. 119. WHICH NATIONS? 121. WHY? DOES THIS HAVE ANY AFFECT ON YOUR CAREER PLANS? YES NO IF NO: GO TO 124. 123. WHAT AFFECT? 1 nineteen FE. 124. 125. 126. 246 Page twenty- COGNATIVE MAP What are the essential characteristics of a science? ALTERNATE: WHAT DISTINGUISHES THE SCIENCES FROM OTHER SUBJECTS, FOR EXAMPLE, FROM LITERATURE, OR ART, 0R PHILOSOPHY? Which are the leading countries in your field? HOW WOULD YOU RANK THESE COUNTRIES? IF HOME COUNTRY IS NOT'MENTIONED IN 125, PROBE 127--OTHERWISE GO TO 128. 128. 130. 127. WHAT ABOUT YOUR HOME COUNTRY? IS IT AMONG THE LEADERS, CLOSE BEHIND, LAGGING EAR BET HIND, OR NOT IN THE PICTURE AT ALL? Has there been any change in the position of your home country during the past decade? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 129. IF NO GO TO 130. 129. HOW HAS IT CHANGED? Do you anticipate any change in the position of your home country in the forseeable future? YES NO '"enty fi‘ 132. 133. 135. 244’ Page twenty-one 131. WHY OR WHY NOT? If you had an outstanding student which country would you send him to for the best possible professional training in your field? Which are the leading journals in your field? 134. WHICH COUNTRIES ARE THEY PUBLISHED IN? Is there any country-or are there any countries- which makes it difficult or impossible for you to learn what its scientists in your field are doing? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 136, 137. IF NO, GO To 138. 136. WHICH COUNTRY(IES)? 137. WHY IS IT DIFFICULT OR IMPOSSIBLE TO LEARN WHAT SCIENTISTS IN THIS COUNTRY(IES) ARE DOING? "' va-one 138. 141. 143. 248 Page twenty-two Is there any country(ies) whose scientists you would not share your work with? IF YES, PROBE 139, 140. IF NO, GO TO 141. 139. WHICH COUNTRY(IES)? F 140. WHY WOULDN'T YOU SHARE YOUR WORK? Are there any conditions under which you would not share your work with scientists in another country(ies)? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 142. IF NO, GO TO 143. 142. WHAT CONDITIONS? In evaluating a scientific statement, journal article, etc., by a scientist in your field do you ever take into account the fact that he is from a particular country--does his nationality affect your evaluation in any way? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 144. IF NO, GO TO 145. 144. HOW DOES THIS ENTER INTO YOUR EVALUATION? ‘ ~ cm twenty-two 145. 147. 149. 249 Page twenty-éhfee Think of all the countries you have worked in and all the scientists you have worked with. ‘which count- ry's scientists would you most prefer to work with? IF PREFERENCE EXPRESSED, PROBE 146. IF NO PREFERENCE, GO TO 147. 14 6 0 WHY ? ‘21" Im'fliln‘f Which country's scientists would you most prefer to socialize with? IF PREFERENCE EXPRESSED, PROBE 148. IF NO PREFERENCE, GO TO 149. 148. WHY? Which persons outside of your field do you consider to be part of your audience-~persons you want to share your knowledge with in one way or another? IF NONE, GO TO 153. OTHERWISE, PROBE 150, 151, 152. 150. PROBE NATIONAL, EI-NATIONAL, MULTI-NATIONAL, INTERNATIONAL. 151. WHICH SEGMENTS IN SOCIETY DO THEY REPRESENT? ‘ twenty-- three 153. 157. 159. 1250 Page twenty-four 152. WHY DO YOU WANT To INCLUDE THEM IN YOUR AUDIENCE? Which persons outside of your field would you explic- itly exclude from your audience? IF NONE, GO TO 151. OTHERWISE, PROBE 154, 155, 156. 154. PROBE NATIONAL, III-NATIONAL, Mun—NATIONAL, INTERNATIONAL 155. WHICH SEGMENTS IN SOCIETY DO THEY REPRESENT? 156. WHY DO YOU’WANT TO EXCLUDE THEM FROM YOUR AUDIENCE? How many persons in your field are there in your home country? 158. HOW MANY DO YOU KNOW PERSONALLY? Does it make any difference to you what country you work in? YES NO "Four . Mfi‘.l."*. .l." .3 ' 161. 163. 164. 166 O 251 Page twenty-five 160. WHY OR WHY NOT? Does it make any difference to you what country you live in? YES NO 162. WHY OR WHY NOT? Think of the best possible conditions for carrying out the work you are interested in. Is there any one country (or countries) in which you would be able to work under approximately such conditions? Are there any limits placed on the kind of work you can do in your home country? IF YES, PROBE 165. IF NO, GO TO 166. 165. WHAT KINDS OF LIMITS? Are there any limits placed on the kind of work you can do here? IF YES, PROBE 167. IF NO, GO TO 168. 167. WHAT KINDS OF LIMITS? "-nty-five ‘ *8; :7-l&.ud.‘uuw ' -'l J a 168. 172. 2252 Page twenty-six Are there any other persons from your home country in this department? YES NO DON'T KNOW IF YES, PROBE 169. IF NO OR DON'T KNOW, GO To 173. 170. DO YOU KNOW THEM PERSONALLY? YES NO KNOW SOME IF YES, OR KNOW SOME, PROBE 171. IF No, GO TO 172. 171. WHAT DID YOU TALK ABOUT THE LAST TIME YOU GOT TOGETHER WITH SOME OR ALL OF THEM? Are there any other persons from your home country at this university? YES NO DON'T KNOW IF YES, PROBE 173. IF NO, OR DON'T KNOW, GO TO 175. 173. DO YOU KNOW THEM PERSONALLY? YES NO KNOW SOME IF YES, OR KNOW SOME, PROBE 174. IF NO GO TO 175. 174. WHAT DID YOU TALK ABOUT THE LAST TIME YOU GOT TOGETHER? ‘tv-six ' “ .‘zL. J; v;‘_ 7. fig, r V“. .Sn‘. n 253 Page twenty-seven 175. Are there any other persons form your home country in your field visiting in the United States? YES NO KON'T KNOW 176. When you retire, or nearing the end of your career, what would you like peOple in general to remember you for? 177. WHICH PERSONS WOULD YOU LIKE TO BE REMEMBERED BY IN PARTICULAR? 178. Some scientists believe that the criteria for truth and verification in science will never change. Do you agree with them? YES NO 179. WHY OR WHY NOT? 180. What would you say are the most productive years for a scientist in your field? 181. If you could change your profession today, would you? IF YES, PROBE 182. IF NO, PROBE 183. :ven 1254 Page twenty-eight 182. WHAT WOULD YOU CHANGE TO, AND WHY? 183. WHY NOT? IF RESPONDENT HAS BEEN ABROAD BEFORE ASK.QUESTIONS 184, 185, 186. IF THIS IS HIS FIRST TRIP ABROAD GO TO 187. 184. 187. 188. Upon returning to your home country did you (or do you usually) discuss your visit with peOple in your field? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 185. IF NO, PROBE 186. 185. WHY? 186. WHY NOT? Have you tried to arrange for one or more of your colleagues and/or students to visit abroad? YES NO Have you tried to arrange visits to your home country for scientists you met abroad? YES NO 255 Page twenty-nine 189. For each of the following types of scientists indicate the extent to which you feel you have something in common with them by virtue of being a scientist. 7'7 Physical scientists GREAT DEAL IN COMMON SOMETHINGS IN COMMON A FEW THINGS IN COMMON, BUT NOT MANY NOTHING AT ALL IN COMMON Biological scientists GREAT DEAL IN COMMON SOMEATHINGS IN COMMON A FEW THINGS IN COMMON, BUT NOT MANY NOTHING AT ALL IN COMMON Social scientists GREAT DEAL IN COMMON SOME THINGS IN COMMON A FEW THINGS IN COMMON, BUT NOT MANY NOTHING AT ALL IN COMMON Dana f‘annfv-n‘i n9, 190. 191. 192. 256 Page thirty Of the following, which best characterizes your present work? a. Specifically related to physical and/or biological problems indigenous to my home country b. Specifically related to economic, social, and/or political problems of my home country c. Specifically related to physical and/or biological problems indigenous to a specific region (e.g., Southeast Asia) d. Specifically related to economic, social, and/or political problems of a specific region (e.g., Southeast Asia) e. Specifically related to physical and/or bio- logical problems affecting the world as a whole f. Specifically related to economic, social and/or political problems which involve all nations 3. Has no relationship to national or geographic boundaries On the basis of your reSponse to the last question, what effect does the character of your work have on the nature of your career, if any? Is it an asset, a liability, or irrelevant to getting ahead in your field, making a name or reputation, etc? PROBE FOR CAREER REFERENT - HOME COUNTRY, UNITED STATES, INTERNATIONAL SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY... GENERATIONS How many generations can you identify in your field in your home country? PROBE FOR REFERENT: IS IT SOCIETAL, OR SCIENTIFIC? 193. 194. 195. 197. 257 Page thirty-one Which generation do you belong to? Are there any significant differences between the generations you have identified? PROBE FOR COMMITMENT To BUILDING A SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY (NATIONAL, BI-NATIONAL, MULTINATIONAL OR INTERNATIONAL) AND CONCERN FOR PROBLEMS OF DEVELOPMENT, MODERNIZATION, AND POST-MODERNIZATION. Do you feel any obligations to the next generation of Scientists in your home country? YES NO 196. WHY OR WHY NOT? CONDITIONS OF WORK 4- During the last five years where have you Spent most of your working hours? a. Laboratory b. Firld (social surveys, geological or geo- graphic surveys, etc.) c. Clinic_______ d. Library._____ e. Office _____ f. Other __ IF MORE THAN ONE OF THE ABOVE IS RELEVANT, RANK.ORDER YOUR RESPONSES MW 1'“ v. \4. 1358 Page thirty-two 198. Indicate the extent to which each of the following has characterized your work over the last five years. a. Theoretical: Definitely characteristic Somewhat characteristic Not characteristic b. Methodological: Definitely characteristic Somewhat characteristic Not characteristic C. Experimental: Definitely characteristic Somewhat characteristic Not characteristic d. Technological (including applied work, research and deveIOpment, etc.): Derinitely character- istic Somewhat characteristic Not Characteristic e. Clinical: Definitely characteristic Some- what characteristic Not characterisitc f. Empirical: Definitely characteristic Somewhat chaacterist Not characteristic 199. During the last five years, how many peOple have you usually worked with on wach of your studies? IF RESPONDENT HAS WORKED ALONE, GO TO 201. OTHER- WISE, PROBE 200. 200. WHAT KIND OF RELATIONSHIP DID YOU HAVE WITH THE PERSON(S) YOU WORKED WITH - WERE THEY COLLEAGUES, TECHNICIANS, SUPERVISORS...? 201. What would you say are the most important tools and resources in your work - things you must have in order to carry out your research? na-Amtr—‘T 232. 203. 204. 259 Page thirty-three How well organized is your field in terms of a body of empirically corroborated hypotheses, systematic the- ories, etc.? Some scientists are working on the forefronts of know- 'T““ ledge, in fields or subfields that are just beginning j to receive attention; others are involved in research i that is peripheral to the main concerns of men in their é field; still other scientists may fit somewhere between these two extremes. How would you characterize your work with reference to your field in general? 1 To what extent does your work entail financial costs requiring large-scale funding (e.g., government funding)? COMMUNITY AND PUBLIC ACTIVITIES (REFERENT IS HOME COUNTRY UNLESS OTHERWISE SPECIFIED) 205. Have you been involved in any non-scientific organiz- ations or activities (for example, as an elected public official, a public lecturer, a civic leader, etc.) at the local level (e.g., in your community) during the last five years? IF YES, PROBE 206-216. IF NO, PROBE 217. 236. WHAT ACTIVITIES AND ORGANIZATIONS, AND IN WHAT CAPACITY? 207. ARE YOU STILL INVOLVED IN THESE ACTIVITIES AND ORGANIZATIONS? Page thirty-three 260 Page thirty-four 208. DO YOU INTEND TO REMAIN INVOLVED? 209. WHEN YOU ARE ENGAGED IN THESE ACTIVITIES DO YOU THINK OF YOURSELF PROMARILY As A /respondent's field/, A SCIENTIST, A CITIZEN...? ~.; I Q 4 g . b fl | 210. D0 OTHERS THINK OF YOU IN THE SAME WAY? I 4'2... (2.1.: m 4‘ 211. WHAT DO COLLEAGUES IN YOUR FIELD THTNK ABOUT YOUR PARTICIPATION IN THESE ACTIVITIES? 212. WHY DO YOU ENGAGE IN THESE ACTIVITIES? 213. BY VIRTUE OF BEING A SCIENTIST, IS THERE ANYTHING THAT ESPECIALLY QUALIFIES you To ENGAGE IN THESE ACTIVITIES? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 214. IF NO, GO To 215. 214. WHAT IN PARTICULAR? Page thirty-four 218. 220. 261 Page thirty-five 215. WHEN PARTICIPATING IN THESE ACTIVITIES DO YOU THINK OF YOURSELF AS A REPRESENTATIVE OF A SPECIFIC GROUP OR ORGANIZATION, FOR EXAMPLE THE SCIENTIFIC COMMUNITY, YOUR HOME COUNTRY...? 216. WHAT IMPACT, IF ANY, HAS YOUR PARTICIPATION IN THESE ACTIVITIES HAD ON YOUR PROFESSIONAL CAREER? GO TO 218. _&A§K: "IGNI-Iir . ‘. O... ~ _ i In. -f'n 217. WHY AREN'T YOU ENGAGED IN SUCH ACTIVITIES? Do you think scientists in general should become invol- ved in the kinds of activities we have been discussing? YES NO 219. WHY 0R WHY NOT? Have you participated in decision-making at the national level during the last five years? YES NO IF YES, PROBE 221-231. IF NO, PROBE 232. 221. WHAT ACTIVITIES IN PARTICULAR, AND IN WHAT CAP- ACITIES? 222. 223. 224. 225. 226. 227. 228. 229. 262 Page thirty-six ARE YOU STILL INVOLVED IN THESE ACTIVITIES? DO YOU INTEND TO REMAIN INVOLVED? WHEN YOU ARE ENGAGED IN THESE ACTIVITIES DO YOU THINK OF YOURSELF PRIMARILY AS A /respondent's field/, A SCIENTIST, A CITIZEN...? DO OTHERS THINK OF YOU IN THE SAME WAY? WHAT DO COLLEAGUES IN YOUR FIELD THINK.ABOUT YOUR PARTICIPATION IN THESE ACTIVITIES? WHY DO YOU ENGAGE IN THESE ACTIVITIES? BY VIRTUE OF BEING A SCIENTIST, IS THERE ANYTH2HG THAT ESPECIALLY QUALIFIES YOU TO ENGAGE IN THESI ACTIVITIES? YES No 1F YES, PROBE 2;9. IF NO, GO TO 230. WHAT IN PARTICULAR? 233. 234. 235. 263 Page thirty-seven 230. WHEN PARTICIPATING IN THESE ACTIVITIES DO YOU THINK OF YOURSELF AS A REPRESENTATIVE OF A SPEC- IFIC GROUP OR ORGANIZATION, FOR EXAMPLE, THE SCI- ENTIFIC COMMUNITY, YOUR HOME COUNTRY»..? 231. WHAT IMPACT, IF ANY HAS YOUR PARTICIPATION IN THESE ACTIVITIES HAD ON YOUR CAREER? GO TO 233. 'n m.:a- .g u 1 232. WHY AREN'T YOU ENGAGED IN SUCH ACTIVITIES? Do you think scientists in general should become invol- ved in the kinds of activities we have been discussing? YES NO Looking over all your experiences here, in other countries and back home, what affect have they had on the way you view man, society and the world? Are there any questions I did not ask which I should? have asked? VISITING FOREIGN SCIENTISTS AND SCHOLARS: A STUDY IN THE SOCIOLOGY OF SCIENCE QUESTIONNAIRE Sal P. Restivo C.K. Vanderpool Department of Sociology Michigan State University Summer 1969 261+ 1. 2. 3. u. 6. 7. 8. 10. 265 Sex: Male Female Age: Marital Status: Single_____ Married_____ Divorced___ Widowedh_~__ Birthplace: Citizenship: __ A_ {II Current Position: Visiting Instructor Visiting Assistant Professor,_______ . Visiting Associate Professor i Visiting Research Associate T Other (specify)__ What is your field?__r' 49 What are your areas of Specialization? Indicate the academic degrees you have earned. Bachelors Degree (or equivalent): Year Country Subject Masters Degree (or equivalent): Year Country Subject Ph.D. (or equivalent) Year Country Subject In the last five years, where have you been employed? a. Organization: Industry Government University Other (Specify)__ b. Country(s) c. Position(s) ll. 12. 13. 1h. 15. 266 (2) Not counting this trip to the U.S., what foreign countries have you visited in the last five years for reasons related to your work? Countries Length of Stays “I Did you interact regularly with foreigners in your f home country? Yes No Why did you come to this country? (Check as many as applicable). a. To do research with colleagues b. To learn about new techniques c. To do research which could not be done at home d. To teach e. Other (specify) —-—v w How did you happen to come to this university? (Check as many as applicable). a. I was invited by American colleagues I met in my home country. b. I was invited by a colleague in the U.S. who knew of my work in my field. 0. A former teacher recommended me for a position. d. Personal initiative. e. A friend of mine was here before f. Other (specify): What do you plan to do after your stay here? a. Stay in the U.S. b. Return home c. Not certain d. Other (specify): "_ 16. 17. 18. 267 (3) What types of activities will you perform after your stay here? a. The same as before I left my home country b. The same as I perform here in the U.S. c. Other (specify) ‘ _ The following list is composed of activities scientists sometimes perform. Please rank each activity in terms of the amount of time and effort you exPend in them while in your home country and now in the U.S. (For example, teaching 2 , Basic research 1. , Organiza- tional activities 3 , etc.). If you haven’t performed the activity please eave the space blank. Home Country U.S. : a. Teaching formal courses and seminars (including preparation time) g b. Basic research 0. Applied research __ d. Teaching-Research (that kind of research carried on with one or more appren- tice researchers for whom this research involvement is part of their formal training). e. Administration within an organization f. Consultant to public and/or private organizations g. Organizational activities in science (editing, membership participation, committee par- ticipation, in science orga- nization, etc.) h. Writing and publication i_ _ii —— “ “ ~— “— 1. Other (specify) ___ Which of the following statements concerning teaching and a career in your field do you agree or disagree with? Use the following rating: 1=Strongly Agree; 2=Agree; 3=Neither agree, nor disagree; h=disagree3 5=Strongly disagree. a. A combination of teaching and research helps a person in my field to be successful. b. Teaching without an emphasis on research is detrimental to a career in my field. 0. Teaching detracts from time and effort that should be spent in research. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 268 (4) In terms of your ability to teach science, do you feel any obligation to or responsibility for the next generation of scientists in your home country? Definitely Somewhat None Comparing your eXperience here in the U.S. and back. home, do you find that there is a difference between American students and students from your home country? Yes No __ If Yes: What is the difference? __ i Do you find that there is a difference in the way you interact with persons in authority here (e.g. Depart~ ment Chairmen, Deans, etc.) as compared with the way you interacted with similar individuals back home? Yes No It If Yes: What is the difference? Has the level of your involvement in work changed since you°ve been here in the following ways? a. Working longer hours Yes No b. Working less that before Yes No 0. More dedication to work Yes No d. Other (specify): Please indicate which of the groups below include mest of the persons you work with. a. Americans 1) in my field 2) not in my field b. Persons from my home country 1) in my field 2) not in my field____ c. Persons from other countries L) in my field 2) not in my field 2h. 25. 26. 269 (5) Please indicate which one of the groups below includes most of the friends you see socially, i.e., away from Work 0 a. People in my field who Work where I do and are from 1) the United States 2) my home country 3) other countries b. People in my field Who work elsewhere and are from if 1) the United States I 2) my home country 3) other countries 0. People not in my field Who work where I do and are from 1) the United States 2) my home country 3) other countries d. People not in my field who work elsewhere and are from 1) the United States 2) my home country 3) other countries Indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement with the following statements. Use the following rating: l=8trongly agree: 2=Agree; 3=Neither Agree nor disagree; H=Disagreeg 5=Strongly Disagree. a. Americans in my field work harder than my colleagues back home. b. My colleagues back home work longer hours than their American counterparts. c. Americans are not as dedicated to their work as my home country colleagues. d. The degree of understanding that Americans have of the problems confronting my field is less than that of my colleagues back home. e. Americans are more organized in their work than my colleagues back home. ____u_ Please indicate the extent to which each of the following has been a part of your scientific work over the past five years. a. Theory construction:great part__some part__no part_ b. Mathematics and , Statistics: great part__some part__no part_ 0. Methodology: great part__some part__no part_ d. EXperimentation: great part__some part__no part_ e. Clinical work: great part__some part__no part_ f. Engineering: great part__some part__no part_ ’1‘. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 270 (6) How important is each of the following in determin- ing your choice of a research problem to work on? a. Problems facing Mankind(e.g, world pOpulation crisis, international conflicts) very important__somewhat important__not at all important__ b. problems facing my home country(e. . economic deve10pment, problems in education) very important__somewhat important__not at all important__ c. scientific problems(e.g.theory, methodology) very important__somewhat important__not at all important__ What do you consider as the single most important factor affecting your choice of a research problem? Do you feel a sense of responsibility for the possible social consequences of your research? Definitely Somewhat Not at all How do you think the research you are doing will affect mankind? a. will definitely be of great benifit b. will definitely have adverse affects c. will definitely have no effect on society in the forseeable future Some scientists and scholars maintain that every scientist and scholar should be directly involved in the decision-making process of their country. Do you agree or disagree with them? Strongly agree___ Agree Neither Agree nor disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree To what extent does your work entail financial costs requiring large-scale funding by major foundations or government agencies? to a great extent to some extent not at all During the last five years where have you spent most of your working hours? If more than one of the following categories is relevant, please clarify by rank ordering them in terms of the amount of time and effort you spent in each setting. a. Laboratory b. Fie1d(socia1 surveys, geological surveys) c. Clinic d. Library e. Office f. Home g. Other(Specify) An - .1.‘fll‘.n‘['&";._o . "|.".:. . _ 3“. 35¢ 36. 37. 38. 39. #0. 41. 271 (7) Please list the leading countries in your field, in ralk order if possible. Leading Country(s) 2nd 3rd 4th 5th If your home country is NOT mentioned in question 3#, please answer this question: Where does your home country fit into the picture? a. Among the leaders_ B. Close behind the leaders_ 0. Lagging behind the leaders“ d. Not at all in the picture_ Do you anticipate any improvement in the position of your home country in the forseeable future? Yes No What country or countries are the scientists you most frequently communicate with from? __ ___ — —— How do you communicate with them? Rank order the following in terms of their importance as a means of communication. a. Letters_ ' b. Telephone c.Associational meetings_ d. Pre-prints/reprints_ e. Personal visits_ f. Other(specify) Is there any country (or countries) whose scientists and scholars it is difficult or impossible for you to communicate regularly and freely with? Yes No If yes: List the countries: What revents regular and free communication? 1) Language 2) Politics 3) Other (specify) Would you accept a permanent job outside of your home country? Yes No Haybe Rate the importance of each of the following items as they would affect your decisions about where you work in terms of the following scale: .=very imfigrtant; 2=somewhat important: 3=hardly important: not impor- 1:81:11: 0 a. Country_ b. Salary_ c. Quality of scientists___ d. Quality* of research facilities_ e. Likes and dislikes of my wife and/or children: f. Other (Specb ify _—___—.-_‘_— #2. #3. ##. #5. #6 . #7. 272 (8) How many papers have you published in your field? 1-5 6-10 11 or more none How many books or monographs have you published in your field? 1'3 #-6______ 7 or more______ none What countries do you usually publish your works in? In evaluating a scientific statement, Journal article, etc. by a person in your field, do you ever take into account the fact that he is from a particular country? Yes No If yes: In what way: Please list the names of the scientific and scholarly societies you belong to and the countries they are located in. Name of Country Name of Society How often do you attend the meetings of these societies? Name of Society every meeting most meetings some meetings no meetings g__ every meeting most meetings some meetings no meetings every meeting most meetings some meetings no meetings every meeting most meetings some meetings no meetings #8 . #9. SO. 273 (9) Are you, or have you been a member of any non—professio- nal organization (civic, charitable, religious, polit— ical, etc.) in your home country? Yes No To what extent do you support the following statements? Use the following rating: l=Strongly agree: 2-Agree; 3=Neither agree nor disagree; #=Disagree; 5=Strongly disagree. a. My country should stay as it is, i.e. it should not change. b. What my country needs most is greater economic development. c. A greater effort in my home country must be placed on a rediscovery of its past. d. The values of science should influence the values and ways of life of the people and leaders of my home country. ' e. The problems confronting my country must ~» be seen as international in nature. f. My country should follow and develop its own course thru history and not copy other nations. g. There should be more international coopera- tion between my country and other nations. HHI What affect have your experiences here and in other countries had on.theway you view peOple, societies, and the world? 1‘ - 14. .""'_’-_A -__.. fin. Appendix B Supplementary Tables 27# Th” 275 Table 1: Number of non-respondents to the Questionnaire: Type of Science and Institution. Type of Science Institution Physical Biological Social Total Wisconsin 22 29 1 52 Purdue 27 13 1 41 ? Em Minnesota* 13 4 2 19 Illinois* 7 14 5 26 Total 69 6O 9 138 *Minnesota and Illinois have less foreign scien- tists on their campuses than Wisconsin and Purdue (see International Infiitute of Education; 1968 . 276 Table 2: Percentage Distribution of Respondents by Their Country of Origin Percentage Percentage Countries Distribution Countries Distribution England 15.3% Norway .9 India 13.9 Spain .9 Japan 12.1 Burma .5 Germany 7.6 Costa Rica .5 China* 6.5 Guyana .5 Australia #.6 Indonesia .5 Canada 3.? Iran .5 Israel 3.7 Ireland .5 Czechoslovakia 2.8 Jordan .5 Korea 2.3 Malaysia .5 Switzerland 2.3 Nepal .5 Italy 2.3 New Zealand .5 Chile 1.9 Okinawa .5 France 1.9 Peru .5 Egypt 1.9 Philippines .5 Pakistan 1.# South Africa .5 Poland i.# Sweden .5 Turkey 1.# Syria .5 Brazil .9 Thailand .5 Colombia .9 Venezuela .5 Greece .9 Yugoslavia .5 Hong Kong .9 Total 100.0% Netherlands .9 (N=222) *The figures are for China unspecified and the Republic of China. Our figures include both categories. Only two respondents were classified as China unspecified. .w‘rm..- .;5'_r‘. - - . e h v. n .1. BIBLIOGRAPHY 277 PM“- e 1-7 Bibliography Apter, David E. 1965 The Politics of Modernization. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Barber, Bernard. 1952 Science and the Social Order. New York: 5 The Free Press. ' Beijer, G. ; 1969 "Brain Drain as a Burden, a Stimulus and a Challenge to European Integration." Pp. #-30 in F. Bechhofer, Population Growth and the Brain Drain. Chicago: Aldine Pub- lishing Company. Bell, Daniel. 1968 "The Measurement of Knowledge and Technology." Pp. 1#5-2#6, in Eleanor B. Sheldon and Wilbert E. Moore (eds.), Indicators of Social Change, New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Boulding, Kenneth. 196# The Meaning of the Twentieth Century. New York: Harper and How. Brooks, Harvey. 1968 The Government of Science. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press. The Committee on The International Migration of Talent. 1970 Modernization and the Migration of Talent. Education and World Affairs. Corwin, R.G. 1961 "The Professional Employee.” American Journal of Sociology, 66: 60#—615. Dedijer, Stevan. 1962 "Measuring The Growth of Science." Science 138 (November): 781-788. DeGre, Gerard. 1955 Science as a Social Institution. New York: Random House. 278 279 de Solla Price, Derek J. 1963 Little Science, Big Science. New York: Columbia University Press. Dupree, A. Hunter. 1957 Science in The Federal Government. New York: Harper and Row. Einstein, Albert. 1950 Essays on Humanism. New York: Philosophical Library. Etzioni, Amitai. 1968 The Active Society. New York: The Free Press. Gerth, Hans and C. Wright Mills. 1953 Character and Social Structure. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World. Gilpin Robert. 196A France in The Age of The Scientific State. Priceton, New Jersey: Priceton University Press. Glaser, Barney G. 1963 "The Local-Cosmopolitan Scientists." American Journal of Sociology 69 (November): 2#9-259. Glass, Bentley. 1965 Science and Ethical Values. Chapel Hill, North Carolina: The University of North Carolina Press. Greenberg, Daniel S. 1967 The Politics of Pure Science. New York: The New American Library. Hagstrom, Warren D. 1965 The Scientific Community. New York: Basic Books Incorporated. Harbison, Frederick and Charles A. Myers. 196# Education, Manpower, and Economic Growth. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hays, William L. 1963 Statistics for Psychologists. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Henle, R.J. 1965 Final Report on The Project To Devise and Test Simplified Adequate Systems of Measuring 280 and Reporting Fincancial, Manpower Facilities, Research and Other Activities in Colleges and Universities. washington: National Science Foundation and National Institute of Health (June). The Institute of Political and Social Studies. 1967 "Indian Scientific Policy II." Pp. 387-#00 in A.B. Shah (ed.), Education, Scientific Policy and Developing Societies. Bombay: Manaktalas. International Institute of Education. 1968 Open Doors. New York: International Institute of Education. Jayasuriya, D.L. N.D. "Ceylonese Research Scientists." Pp. 227- 3#8 in Surajit Sinha (ed), Science and Technology in Relation to Cultural Values and Institutions of South and Southeast Asia: India and Ceylon. Unpublished Manuscript. Kaplan, Norman and Norman W. Storer. 1968 "Scientific Communication." In International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences.v New York: Macmillan. Kornhauser, William. 1962 Scientists in Industry. Berkely: University of California Press. Kroche Jr., E.A. 1958 “The Changing Role of The Chinese Intel- lectual." Comparative Studies in Society and History, 1: 23-25. Loomis, Charles P. and Zone K. Loomis. 1965 Modern Social Theories. Princeton: D. Van Nostraud Company. Marcson, Simon. 1960 The Scientist in American Industry. Priceton Princeton University Press. Merton, Robert K. 1938 1957 “Science, Technology and Society in Seven- teenth Century England." OSIRIS IV: 360-632. "Science and the Social Order." Pp. 537-5#9, in Robert K. Merton (ed.), Social Theory and Social Structure, rev. ed., New York: The Free Press. 1965 Miller 196$ Needham, Joseph. Science and International Relations. Oxford: re Blockwell Scientific Publications. 1949 Parsons, Talcott. 1951 Problem of The 'Role of Ideas'.” pp. 326— 281 "The Ambivalence of Scientists.” Pp. 112- 132 in Norman Kaplan (ed.), Science and Society, Chicago: Rand McNally and Co. Delbert C. and William H. Form. Industrial Sociology. New York: Harper and Row. "Belief-System and The Social System: The 383, in Talcott Parsons, The Social System, New York: The Free Press. .' ' 1' Pp!” Parthasarati, Ashok. 1967 ”India's 'Brain-Drain' and International Norms.“ International Education and Cultural Exchange, U.S. Advisory Com- mission, Summer. Pelz, D.C. and F.M. Andrews. 1967 Scientists in Organizations. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Porter, John. 1968 “The Future of Upward Mobility." American Sociological Review, 33: 5-19. Price, Donald K. 1967 Reif, F. 1965 The Scientific Estate. Cambridge: Belknap Press. "The Competitive World of The Pure Scientist." Pp. 133-1#5 in Norman Kaplan (ed.), Science and Society, Chicago: Rand McNally and Co, Ribeiro, Darcy. 1967 “Universities and Social Development." Pp. 3#3-381 in Seymour N. Lipset and Aldo Solari (eds.), Elites in Latin America. New York: Oxford University Press. Shils, Edward. 1968 ”Introduction." Pp. V-XIV in Edward Shils (ed.), Criteria fbr Scientific Development. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Tech— nology Press. 282 Sinha, Surajit. 1970 "Indian Scientists: The Socio-Cultural and Organizational Context of Their Pro- fessional Environment.“ Pp. 159-225 in Surajit Sinha (ed.), Science, Technology and Culture. New Delhi, India: Research Council on the Study of Culture in In- ternational Relations. _ . Storer, Norman W. 1966 The Social System of Science. Chicago: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Strauss, Anselm and Lee Rainwater. 1962 The Professional Scientists. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company. Swisher Jr., Earle. 1958 ”Chinese Intellectuals under Western Impact, 1838--1900.'| Comparative Studies in Society and History, 1: 26-37. United Nations. 1963a. Science and Technology for Development: Report on the United Nations Conference on The Application of Science and Technology for the Benefit of The Less Developed Areas. Science and Planning: Vol. VII. New York: United Nations. 1963b. Science and Technology for Development: Re- port of The United Nations Conference on The Application of Science and Technology for The Benefit of Less Developed Areas. Plenary Proceedings: Vol. VIII. New York: United Nations. Useem, John and Ruth Hill Useem. 1955 The Western Educated Man in India. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. 1967 I'The Interfaces of a Binational Third Cul- ture: A Study of The American Community in India." The Journal of Social Issues, 23, 1, 130-13#. 1968 ICross-Cultural Studies of The Scientific Community.” Unpublished Manuscript. Useem, John, Ruth Hill Useem, and John Donoghue. 1963 “Men in the Middle of the Third Culture: The Roles of American and Non-Western Peoples in Cross-Cultural Administration.” Human Organi- zation, 22: 169-179. 283 Vanderpool, Christopher K. 1966 "Conceptions of Science and its Inter- relationships with Society." Michigan State University: Unpublished M.A. Thesis. Waisanen, Frederick E. 1969 ”Actors, Social Systems, and The Moderni- zation Process.“ Paper Presented at the Carnegie Seminar, Indiana University: April, 1968, Bloomington Indiana: Depart- ment of Government, Indiana university. Waisanen, Frederick E. and Hideya Kumata. 1969 “Education, Functional Literacy and Parti- cipation in Development.“ Paper Presented to The Annual Meeting of The Society for Applied Anthropology: April, Mexico City, Mexico. BIBLIOGRAPHIC ADDENDUM Form, William H. and Delber C. Miller. 1960 Industry, Labor, and Community. New York: Harper and Row Inkeles, Alex. 1960 "Industrial Man: The Relation of Status to Experience, Perception and Value," American Journal of Sociology, LXVI, No. 1 (July) PD. 1-31-