43 3:7- "‘3 Kg} p: 6 4.9 A v‘. .0-.- a ul- . I. "A ‘ .. us “ "0‘ x s ‘ q ‘ a 9 II" I” .4 1 ABSTRACT LIFE STYLES AND ETHICAL VALUES OF MEN AND WOMEN ON TELEVISION, 1960-1974 Criticism of the values associated with pOpular culture has frequently assumed that: l) the mass society is "lowbrow" and demands media products of low value, 2) highly commercialized or "formula" literature and media are inherently bad, or 3) the values contained within the content of popular culture are of a low orden, a tragedy considering its popularity and assumed influ- ence. Other critics, however, have studied the values contained in popular media in order to understand better individual works, popular art forms and the cultures which produced these popular works and art forms. The present study has as its goal to determine the values contained in television in order to better understand how TV functions as an art form and what TV is indicating about the values of our culture. Specifically, the purpose of the dissertation is to determine, using Lawrence Kohlberg's scale of values develOpment, the values portrayed by television charac- ters in dramatic style prime time programs. Prior to Lawrence Kohlberg, philOSOphers attempting to define "value" have claimed 1) that value is located in the environment, merely awaiting discovery by l n av :..:9... a a u.- .--.'— un- . o-o'no. -. ' C. ‘ C.... 9-. .- 3... ..-_ - .- . . ‘ .‘Ou .‘.-- ...‘. n. u ‘. ‘ s .. s' "\ a... :' ...'. “‘-c b. . a 1-. ..§.. 4a.- ’ 5...- ...~. ‘—.. O x *a z 4 . L 0.. ~ 9... " ‘ o . . - __ ‘.: '.¢I ' 5....” 1 .. r .00 ’«r. Q . 3-...‘ O- |‘... .a :v-. . s~._..s. . O ' h . ‘Ao -..O- b “\ ‘. V: r. -- I §;.z~, -.~"gr ' a": ‘ C - I- . u i- Q n s.” ‘ I‘ ‘~ p ‘ "“.‘Cv ‘_.. "s I‘- . \- “ . 'h a ‘ . ‘ J‘- "u '3; A w s . u o"‘ "q " v‘ n \‘ - U .‘_ n ‘O I t" 'A ' uf'y. QVg “-. '1. .t‘s‘ V \ g D ‘.. 19;. “‘.". - the human mind; or 2) that values are the internal and subjective creation of the individual valuer. These approaches are mutually exclusive, and philosophers in both camps have failed to produce pratical tools for values analysis. Modern behavioral scientists, however, claim they can predict and even direct value changes. Since the assumption of many mass media critics that the media transmits and influences values is essentially behaviorist in nature, behaviorist theory seems a logical place to begin an analysis of values associated with television. In the early 1970's, a large-scale behavioristic study of the relationship between televised violence and aggressive behavior in children and adolescents was conducted under the auspices of the United States Surgeon General's office. Though critics have succeeded in minimizing the study's public impact, findings of the study indicated that violence viewing and aggressive behavior are significantly correlated. This finding lends considerable support to the proposition that mass media both reflects and can influence human values and behavior. Since behavioristic studies can only account for a portion of the relationship between media and certain values associated with specific behavioral traits, however,4a more comprehensive means of analyzing the values ppojected by television is needed. .- .» i,". v..-_ . § :4 ...- I. k: .. CDC-.. 0 ~~o . ~ . ....' . .. = H ‘ a. . O-‘o'-o-- an ’ .l. -- . - v ' g . ."QI. . . O O .5-_ ~ -_ 0 lg... -.-. . a .- "‘0‘. “'~c.~. . T... O. ' "‘ o..- ~ ..,"c C " -- ‘o- ‘ 'i..: e ‘ . Q 0 ’u.: §. ... Ccct“ -_ o.. \ -.. cs 1'. ‘ 7.1.... 0 ~. .t‘“. 5g ‘. vs. .. Q ‘C 'D. ~-‘ (-0 . ‘tu. . -. F- I H R.- o.,‘ .. 6. . u to: Ha. '- “ . ..‘:‘-: F l v 1 v... “5.3: .-.c ' V u.5.:’ 3‘ I ‘ I '0 g “‘1' “v ‘ Q h I o 3 :-- . ‘ . ~‘g “‘5 ' b - ‘5 x. n. \ ~._‘t_ ‘l'...~ .. Based on longitudinal, culturally unbiased studies, Lawrence Kohlberg has postulated the existence of a universal, hierarchical scale of values develop- ment. Kohlberg's scale of six stages was derived in part from Jean Piaget's earlier theory of the structural- developmental nature of human intelligence, which holds that both the human organism and the environment are mutually essential to the valuing process. Since each of Kohlberg's six stages is characterized by certain views of life, he has found it possible to interrogate subjects regarding their reasoning process and their approach to life and determine the stage of values development which a given subject has reached. The dramatic nature of television programs has the effect of placing characters in situations which reveal their ability to assume various approaches to life (or sets of values). Since Kohlberg's model allows us to identify the stage of values development of a subject by observing his or her reaction to a hypothetical social dilemma, it is also deemed possible to similarly analyze a given character on some TV show by tabulating the stage levels of the character's responses to social situations he or she faces. Specific programs and.characters to be studied are identified by selecting characters on the most popular prime time programs. Although the main focus is on the 1974 r. x. n. v. .v .w. ~\» 3. ... r. u” .. .L . . 5. Q.” o . w. .. .3 u. «id 9: u. o. v. o. o. 4‘ s. r. o. v. 5» n. to. .2 2. .1 a. ... t. . u v. .5. a c s . G» u I . ... a. r. u. .u. v. .v .‘ o -.. . .. .uu 4 5 n o . . . v s I. a u. I 3- 3. s: . .r. - k I. . . o . L” "n. a» a... “A s . .3 .o .u. v. n g a. . . I .(q r. -\5 . u. .-. . o 2. cc. .. .. .u. a. u u: .. . . an. .u as . . .u. “I” 3.. ~.. $- ._ .q. .H .o .. .. 2- .. u. g. .u q 2 a u .u. . . h§ - . I. .aw ‘4‘ . ~ .u. v. n. 2- .~ -. u .- qr . .u :s ...u o u o I. D b .7. O b O u o... s s D . A‘s cot O to. I . 1 .. v television season, scripts and reruns of popular pro- grams between the years 1960 and 1973 are also analyzed to give historical perspective to the analysis. Characters from the three leading programs in each prime time program category are analyzed in detail to illustrate the technique. In comedy leader All in the Family, Archie Bunker represents the continuation of a long trend of popular Stage 2 (preconventional) comic characterizations. Kohlberg's "conventional" morality (or Stages 3 and 4 on his values development scale), portrayed on All in the Family by Edith, Gloria, and Mike, provides the challenge to Archie's world view that produces comedy on the program. Family drama such as The Waltons, on the other hand, depends on an under- lying Stage 3 conventional morality which is shared by all the principal characters. Drama/adventure programs feature a superhero (and rarely a superheroine) who embodies, inflexibly, the Stage 4 norms of our society. Steve McGarrett of the leading drama/adventure program Hawaii Five-O is analyzed as typical of this group of characters. As detailed in Chapters 6 and 7, the overall findings of this study indicate that TV portrays pri- marily conventional morality (Stage 3 and Stage 4 on the Kohlberg'scale) which Kohlberg believes is the norm for our society. Frequency studies show that typically these conventional values are portrayed on drama and I" [-1’” --o .Q-c-.. v ~'. 4 o p ago—An .U'.to.-o -o 0---' ,v,. r .- no... D... . b ~~.~... - -‘ . u ' V. ~‘I‘. .0vVQ-o‘ I I '1' ' I I 'v. no v...,. ':.:- O. . . :u' ': .. :0. . '0' .. ---.. I. “ . Q... - o ‘.-.n: -"O._ \u .y-‘.‘ 'I- ‘ . '9 b‘ . ‘. . o s... ~.4 fl... ‘ ‘4‘ '5. .-‘.‘ . Q.. ?-'_~. . 'g, .. ~ C~“ drama/adventure programs by single, professional males in their prime-of-life age range, though women in comedy programs tend to portray conventional values as well. Since by combining Marshall McLuhan's analysis of print versus electric values with Kohlberg's value scale it appears that the values conveyed by the TV medium itself are also conventional, television is considered to be a strong force for conventional morality in our society. In addition, since conventional values are modal for TV, America's most popular art form, Kohlberg's hypothesis that conventional morality is modal for the U.S. population gains support from the present study. Finally, the present study indicates that analyzing the values of characters, using the Kohlberg scale,can yield a rewarding new understanding of dramatic art forms. '0‘ .0- t V." ..-J 1'. go. do .1 .s s. p... v. C. .n— uyv u: .u .\§ LIFE STYLES AND ETHICAL VALUES OF MEN AND WOMEN ON TELEVISION, 1960-1974 by u\& Kathryn P? Weibel A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of English (American Studies) 1975 This dissertation is dedicated to Steven Merle Freeman ii I C v. .c he .n. 2. .- —.. \Au .§ we. I. 9. .H a: 3.. .. :. “I ... a. 3 we ... .J ... . . . . r. T. .. . S a. a. S a. a. .3 . . 3. a. L n E .t .. a L. A; u. I. a. . . L. t. e. w. ;. ... .2 .. a A. .3 .u s. .. L. v. 1. a: ab .3. .I ..« re .3 . . ... .3 . . s . ~ .. u u. s I . . Um .Q ml . . a. 3‘ 3. 1n . . .a . ~ 5 z. 3 Eu .5 v a 2. . . . n . .. a. a. . . e 7. 3‘ S . .. . .. 3. .. ”I o. .. ... .7 .. .... a. a; :4 o Av .u .N a" . i h... n: . .... ... .5 .5. . . S .5. I h. . . 1. . e n .n s . . v - a n .n In 2 .Lu No. no. .5... .n... ulo. up... H... MW u N u... n. s .o e: \ph‘ hie v- u‘ 0 .I. 34-; ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Several people encouraged me to undertake this dissertation project despite the fact that it sought to combine a rather new and difficult body of theory to a new and yet controversial medium. These included, first and foremost, my advisor, Professor Russel B. Nye. As a pioneering scholar in three new academic disciplines over the past forty years, Dr. Nye has learned to welcome risk, diversity and interdisciplinary perspective as the seeds of creative learning. His encouragement and incisive criticism were invaluable to the present project. Much more than this, however, his example as an ethical professional and human being will be a standard for me in the years to come. Special thanks also go to my mate, Steven M. Freeman, for his continued faith and encouragement and for assisting with the collation of the statistical data. My good friend Marion Welsh Van Winkle introduced me to Lawrence Kohlberg and spent many hours in conver- sation with me discussing nuances of the theory and hashing over ideas for this project. Professor Victor Howard, Director of the American Studies Program, gave me my first opportunity to develop a women's studies course. This course was iii Q ‘ I 3......- ‘="= an:- a OO‘D. ,. .- A' .- O.b‘o—-.n.. . . ‘ ‘OA -. . . O t I o... .‘. p o . . Q .- ‘ ~ -........_“_ - ‘ -. ' "t "’ -u e..‘. 0!. ' . a. :.;'.z. ‘c.:-. -‘ .0; D'A‘;~. ?‘ u-.. -. ‘ u“ ‘- .u.‘ ‘3'... .‘. .c ~ I ~. . ._ I“...':.‘. a turning point in my graduate career. Professor Howard's help and encouragement have been invaluable. Thanks are also due to Professor Barbara Steidle (Department of History) and Professors George Hough, III and the late W. Cameron Meyers (both Department of Journalism) for supervising my work in the cognate areas of my American Studies program. Professors Howard, Steidle and Hough all made thoughtful critiques of the dissertation which will be useful in expanding the project. The dissertation was expertly typed by The Secretariat (Jean and Walt Whipple) of Ann Arbor, Michigan. iv I... .o‘ (I. 9 \~ “I“ . ..“~J . “r. \ b~~0 ‘ "'§‘ ‘ “ I- ‘ A". , .._.. A c~~ s... I“: .u a. r... . :-‘..P§ §J¥'; m..‘ "u 5 “‘~ TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER 1. VALUES AND THE MASS MEDIA: SOME PAST CRITICAL ASSUMPTIONS . . 2. WHAT ARE VALUES? SOME PHILOSOPHICAL DEFINITIONS OF VALUE 3. BEHAVIORISM AND MEDIA ANALYSIS: THE SURGEON GENERAL'S REPORT ON TELEVISION AS A CASE STUDY OF THE BEHAVIORISTIC APPROACH . . . . . . 4. LAWRENCE KOHLBERG'S THEORY OF VALUES DEVELOPMENT 0 C O O C O O O 5. METHODOLOGY FOR APPLYING KOHLBERG'S THEORY TO AN ANALYSIS OF TELEVISION CHARACTERS . . . . . . . 6. A STRUCTURAL VALUES ANALYSIS OF PRIME TIME TELEVISION SERIES: FOCUS ON ALL IN THE FAMILY, THE WALTONS, AND HAWAII FIVE-O . . 7 THE KOHLBERG VALUES AND FREQUENCY OF APPEARANCES OF PRIME TIME TELEVISION CHARACTERS, BY SEX, MARITAL STATUS, OCCUPATION, AGE, AND PROGRAM CATEGORY, 1960-1974 . . 8. SUMMARY, SPECULATIONS, CONCLUSIONS, AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FURTHER RESEARCH APPENDICES O O O O O O O C O O O 0 O O 0 . REFERENCES 0 O O O O O O C O O O O O O 0 0 vi viii 29 6O 80 115 155 191 230 253 273 .,.' 5“ v... z‘ 3‘ R» r. h. by a: .: .Q .1. ... .... .... . z. \r. A» ..~ v. .t 4. at 4‘ .2 u- C h on ,- Q h o. —h\ i... a: o I. a C» .- ',~- I..— .'§Il...- i~ g I“. ¢l $.1- 9A. 9B. 9C. 10. LIST OF TABLES From L. Kohlberg's Theory of Moral Judgment: Classification of Moral Judgment into Levels and Stages of Development . . . . . . . . . . . . . Piaget Stages of Cognitive Development . Major Concerns and Characteristics of Kohlberg's Stages of Values Development . 1974-75 Season Programs Selected for Analysis, by Nielsen Rank and Nielsen Rating . . . . . . . . . . . . . Programs in the Fall 1974 Television Season by Rating and Program Category . . Top Nielsen-Rated Programs for 1960-1973, by Program Category . . . . . . . . . . . Composit Listing of Top Nielsen—Rated Programs, 1960-1973, by Program Type, and Availability of Reruns or Scripts . . . . Average Kohlberg Values of TV Characters and Average Frequency of Appearance of the Characters by Sex, by Marital Status and by Occupation for the 1974 Television Season . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Average Kohlberg Values of TV Characters for Comedy by Sex and Marital Status for the Years 1962 Through 1974 . . . . . . . Average Kohlberg Values of TV Characters for Drama by Sex and by Marital Status for the Years 1962 Through 1974 . . . . . Average Kohlberg Values of TV Characters for Drama/Adventure by Sex and Marital Status for the Years 1962 Through 1974 . All Characters in 1974 Sample by Age, Kohlberg Value, and Marital Status . . . vi 82 93 98 120 121 124 128 194 196 197 198 205 .66 nc-Ao nu. .‘:.y. . '0u‘». .t . CD. is..- ‘o .0. a Q .5. .4. . a O b nus . Q rs U I. .4‘ h . v” H: :5 I». a... 13...: a: .3 I. u u u n.. 11. 12. 13. 14A. 14B. 15. 16. A Comparison Between Kohlberg Values of TV Characters and the Viewing Population by Sex and by Program Category: 1974 . Distribution of the U.S. Population by Sex, by Marital Status and by Occupation for Census Year 1970 . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of TV Characters by Sex and Marital Status and Program Category for the Years 1962 - 1974 . . . . . . . TV Population: 1974, by Age, Sex and Marital Status . . . . . . . . . . . . . U.S. Population: 1974, by Age, Sex and Marital Status . . . . . . . . . . . Data for 1974 Season Analysis . . . . . Data for Historical Analysis . . . . . . Vii 207 217 220 224 225 254 257 LIST OF APPENDICES A. PRIMARY DATA TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . 253 B. THE PROCEDURE USED TO AGGREGATE DATA BY VARIABLE OF INTEREST . . . . . . . 262 viii an “aw-W; u v: ". ‘. a 9.2 '. "‘.‘It- A P >- ." ‘~.-l.a .0 ... ‘.~ ‘ U ‘ r' -. -.~ .b“‘ ""~... ‘-.- \.. ‘ v- h" 0... A ‘I. ' ‘ n. .A a: I... .V .'- .v".~‘ .. b u.“ -- S- "I .4 .- -.,.~,‘ “a'. \ on w “'..J._ O. .‘c ‘ I ‘ I.._. o v... to.” 5:: .‘ , . '§ . ‘. . . I- covk ‘5 ¥. 9 I i'. .-. \n. “.e It I lb' r'\ CHAPTER 1 VALUES AND THE MASS MEDIA: SOME PAST CRITICAL ASSUMPTIONS As a recent surveyor of American media habits put it, "Television is still the number one 'Spectator sport' of the American public." The average television consumer spends 1200 hours per year in front of his set, as com- ;pared to less than 400 hours per year spent reading all ;printed media combined. There are one hundred million 'television sets in the United States, with 95 percent <3f all homes having at least one.1 Unquestionably, tele- ‘Vision is the number one mass medium of our mass society, and.the facts of television consumption alone lead to the inescapable conclusion that television must be saying something about our society, and about artistic expression in general. In particular, since television is the medium that is closest to the public, it seems logical to assume that the human values portrayed by television bear a Significant relationship to the values of the viewing Public. ‘With the goal in mind of discovering this rela- tionship, it will be the purpose of this study to analyze and measure the values contained in television content as they are revealed in the actions and attitudes of prime time TV characters. These values will then be com— pared to those of the viewing public, or "mass society." ‘ Q r' .. ‘ .~ - ‘ .... :2»... _.. . O r “H . -. Chi, :_,_,_ . VI ‘ Q... ;~‘ '- .M....‘..-‘. '. 1.. . - . _ ‘\ “- 0‘.. Q... . o ‘- :- = v‘..:' ~. I p -;v :V. ._" n. “. .'.. ~V\.I‘Q ~‘ A. boou.~.: '. . b; ;'I.-'-' . .u..l.3.‘“.' ‘ ~,_ . .. a . F.... .5. h...." :. v- s.-«,-- cunt. a.-: .‘: I‘h ". ’v~ ‘ cuu ...g ‘ ‘ .- a‘t. o .- t‘§‘§5~s a " - ‘ “‘=’ ‘ .‘ ‘ J .4. ‘- ~ - ‘~:‘":~. ‘ . \=~ an‘ ~ ‘ O ‘. .‘ls ‘.‘ ~.' ‘ Q n O n.‘ ‘; ‘ at 5-.e z“- ‘ v‘ o s. v- .' ”t" as .-. s. : ~H . § .‘ :‘bv‘Q v~ ‘ ‘ § i‘:r ‘ h v- C ‘..- \~‘ - . 'N I ~..~' ‘u . l "“3“ 2. \ “3.”. L‘- ¢.- ~- ‘I- ‘ Er;-_ s I.: .‘ 5 . \‘\..:s‘-. ‘. ‘ '1 ' '2 $§ gag ‘3: ~.. qu‘ ~ A serious study of the most popular art form seems appropriate at the present time. In his recent book, Popular Culture and High Culture, Herbert Gans explains that, particularly in times of low prestige for intellectuals, negative criticism of popular art forms has flourished. The focus of the criticism has varied, says Gans, but it is always leveled at the most popular new art form of any given period. In the 1950's and 1960s, criticism of this type was primarily directed against television, though in the late sixties, mass media critics turned away from the media per se to concentrate their attacks on the morals of the growing youth culture. With the prestige of the intelligentsia again waning in the wake of the Nixon administration, however, Gans predicts a revival of the mass society criticism for the later 1970's.2 Since television continues to be the newest and most popular mass medium, it seems certain that, if Gans' predictions are correct, TV will come in for the brunt of the attack. The critique itself, how- ever, as this chapter will show, is as old as the history of popular culture. A number of critics of literary forms of mass media, including Ian Watt, Leo Lowenthal, Raymond and Alice Bauer, Marshall McLuhan, Russel Nye and others, have emphasized the importance of the discovery of printing as a primary historical factor in creating both mass media and the mass society. As the Bauers note, II’ '5'. rv .. : v -“ a... . v'r' .- g' 5". o-vuo .. . C‘t-A. g.. u a 'U-ouu 0-. g._ I'- . . 0|... . ... . . “In; .. ‘i‘oun..' ” . .'.. Du . .._,L _.: EC. U u 'u. .‘Q' ‘. In. uscu...:-_ .h- \ .vi.“‘ —. 0 D....'.= I. _ . 9-.. Q. ~.: ‘V'A o .u. ' . .‘~. . “\s ‘c... ‘ r -~ a.“ 4 . 0.. > O o.. 4 .Q‘ ‘ h-c.=‘ .- .n’ I. ‘_ “ . \.~ .M Q. \ ~t‘ A a“ e“ ,. ‘Vo. n y «u - . §;- "‘u VI. “’ “Va ._ C E“ b q \ U' q “.‘u. V- I. ”av... ‘ -‘:kl s :‘M. R .‘\a a: \v‘ 5 ~ .12.. ever after this discovery, the economics of mass communi- cation demanded that such media reach a successively broader audience.3 The attempt to reach a larger audience, however, has always been subject to criticism from the established artistic intelligentsia. This criticism has taken place on essentially three fronts: l) criticism of the mass society and the media products it prefers, 2) criticism of the technological form of mass media products, and 3) criticism of the values con- tained in the content of various mass media products. Although this last area of criticism is the primary concern of this dissertation, the first two are closely related to it, and a review of the issues involved in the critique is important to an understanding of my rationale in choosing to emphasize the values contained in media content. In the 1950's, David Bell surveyed twentieth century criticism of the mass society to determine its common perspective.4 He identified several well known critics of mass society, including Ortega y Gasset, Karl Mannheim, Karl Jaspers, Paul Tillich, Gabriel Marcel and Emil Lederer. Bell noted that all these individuals brought an aristocratic, Catholic or existentialist perspective to their criticism. In more general terms, these critics of mass society operated out of a tradi- tional, authoritarian perspective. They were concerned ..| l . .. V. o .- . . . q . .. . .. . r. .J. in ‘» .... u... .d .... c .t .3 LL .9. a. .. .. u; a .. at s.. a. p Q. ~. .. .ww Q. :5 On. a... .0 5. vQ a» «b ‘5 e 8- a;. $. A. 1‘ h. b. \ Q ANN N\\ ~ Q .N‘ \\\ . § .r- .154 -. r. . . s a?” \hh in uh.» o c p. ‘e .- .u —u {s .n v. . . .q .3 .3 .u. AW L» h: Vs s» .n .u ..n n . u . . . . . . . . .! s r .. .. .n- “V Mel. .C .3 .y a.- .nn 0; ... .. u. an .I s.» .1. .Q ‘a -. ..v at» .5. . s a. r; P; a» PP .C sakv s. .. . . .u .u \v .»~ 0 . .\~ in .s a .3 .4‘ .u u .3 ..u o. a» v .. I. Z. s v. a a. an. . o M .. | .u. n. A 9.5 . ‘ nub o «a. t. A. .A 2‘ .s b. g... nu .0. mu . .. .J. I .3 :\ ..... . . u. . o a» n. .a .3 on 9 ~\. a! ...,,N.. I o ..4. u L. .,Q..hu.h.5n’ “'- .‘J\.,.b:," . . d. with the mechanization of society, which they thought brought a kind of equality that leveled individuals to a lowest common denominator. According to Bell, these critics were concerned that not even a few persons (a cultural elite) would be able to achieve a sense of individual self in a mechanized age. Central to this fear regarding loss of individual- ism was a belief that a general breakdown in cultural, moral and aesthetic values was in process.7 Bell sum- marized the critics' view of the causes and present status of the "mass society" and the nature of its vulnerability in this manner: The revolutions in transportation and commun- nications have brought men into closer contact with each other and bound them in new ways: the division of labor has made them more interdependent; tremors in one part of society affect all others. Despite this greater interdependence, however, individuals have grown more estranged from one another. The old primary group ties of family and local community have been shattered; ancient parochial faiths are questioned; few unifying values have taken their place. Most important, the critical standards of an educated elite no longer shape opinion or taste. As a result, mores and morals are in constant flux, relations between individuals are tangential or compartmentalized rather than organic. At the same time greater mobility, spatial and social, intensifies concern over status. Instead of a fixed or known status symbolized by dress or title, each person assumes a multiplicity of roles and constantly has to prove himself in a succession of new situations. Because of all this, the individual loses a coherent sense of self. His anxieties increase. There ensues a search for new faiths. The I'D-l . _ a .u- . I. I _ . I ~ .. .u 2. .. :— u.. .7 a. .. 2. w .3 .w. ..... .. .5 .. .. .. ..I a» .. y. .n .3 u“ . .H .3 L. .. ~u .. C. .. .C 2. .a. .. Ln .. ~ 4 “I S 3. ‘t .3 .. a. e I n. v. c: in QR. .- e. on .u. .‘u .\u .c v. ,1 nIh «Id ”I .hh .a‘ 2 :u .o .n .~ u' . u .s .. a: o e ‘C :- \I. «4. ..» V . a o» v. S ’6‘ “‘8 EH» (a 7.. ab E» A v .5 »t a» E; . .1. x... .u .5» .. ... .. nos . - ~§ a .: stage is thus set for the charismatic leader, the secular messiah, who, by bestowing upon each person the semblance of necessary grace, and of fullness of personality, supplies a substitute for the older unifying belief that the society has destroyed. 5 As Bell went on to point out, the criticism of mass society consisted of two broad parts. First, there was the judgment regarding the anxieties common in modern living, which Bell considered fairly accurate. Second, there was what he calls a "presumed scientific statement" about the causes of the mass society, which involved industrialization and the demand of the masses for equality. Though industrialization and the demand for equality by the masses are facts which cannot be denied, Bell rejects the notion that they are responsible for the particular quality of modern life that the critics find so frightening. In particular, he warns that "when one seeks to apply the theory of mass society analytically it becomes very slippery." That being the case, Bell turns to an alternative explanation of the mass society theories, finding that they derive directly from the prejudices inherent within the critics themselves. Bell notes the influence of the "dominant con- servative tradition of Western political thought" upon the critics. Beginning with Aristotle's Politics, democracy has been equated with the rule of the mob, which, it is assumed, is easily swayed by demagogues and ’ C O'.. O; ., , n-o-o o-.. 0. . no. :r.. ". I I- t on. #0-“... . 'II ' .O‘ai' \- .‘ a "\ A... ‘ _ Us "‘.: ' - I. k I -. b N: '->-.. . "'. I . _ an... . . . :“ n ch- “¢ u .4 c U. o— ‘\ .- -:‘~ :A' ' 9“ .‘ .3 ‘-o --. c. I.. . ' '~ ‘C‘ ‘I a 0-”... . ' “a A- ~ ‘ "‘ - u... . I O o y s" “ \ ~ . a‘ ‘ ‘ 5 . Sz‘ Ps‘i ‘ In ‘ v- 5 p, n; u ”c v. p b . cv“‘ .. A a, a- v ‘ ~ .h‘ \- must fall ultimately into the hands of tyranny. Thus, the French Revolution "transplanted" the image of the mindless masses into modern consciousness. Since then, cries for social and political equality have created in the minds of conservative (especially Catholic) observers of society an almost paranoic fear that in an "equal" society all established political, social and religious dogma (what Bell calls "traditional values") must neces-. sarily be destroyed. Expanding the base of democracy means for these critics increased public emotionalism, which will surely unleash irrational, possibly revolu— tionary forces. In other words, critics of the mass society fear for the continuance of the established order as they know it, no doubt from a simple concern for self-survival. As Leo Lowenthal convincingly argues in his case study of the birth of the popular novel in eighteenth century England, the first concern of the established intelligentsia (including both authors and critics) was 6 The concern was that the their own self-preservation. newly literate masses, incapable of distinguishing between good and bad art, would purchase only bad art, thereby forcing the elite practitioner out of business. This fear proved insubstantial, of course. As Ian Watt points out, the capitalistic, open market in printing and book selling supported far more artists of all levels - o ~rv .. 1 n-..... ‘ I In 9' ‘ “ ' on. .- . ~ Piano-Iv.‘ .t- , o .‘O"§~ - ‘ c u ‘ ‘ "vuoo‘nu' . It In. _ p . 50'-n.- _. o . . . . I Q, . \‘ :- A. In. ...‘. f I... . . .‘~ .;a. : . 3: ~-~ -.d“' ~§ I y‘ D. ’ I I. ‘ .n' :-‘~;... ~ va.‘.. J - . -- 3 o.._ _ t. H . . . . '>; n ‘_.R — “ I‘u... ‘2‘“. ' O u..-‘ ' .. . D. ‘ ..‘ 5§.E E:' I. ‘9‘: c v;;...‘ - ““.-ai ' su. ~ . J- ~ no. _‘ I: g ." ‘u a c... in o h - . . - " “ J‘- t “ §.. .~ , in. ., ‘ n... n A It: 6“. a b . Q “ .0 g. . U.‘ a . .’:-. ~“‘~F‘Afi_ I.-:‘ ‘ I 5.‘ ~ . x...:.‘.. ‘~. 3. ‘ 5 I t‘.‘ \: ‘- I s u.- “‘¢ ‘1‘ ‘3. ‘- s.“;;‘ ~‘l Mb. - Q-":> ‘ ‘O u“‘. -‘-~‘~~ ‘s ‘1- . ‘ M . ‘ I .‘ ‘ \~ ‘fi ‘3 :5- ‘~‘ 1 . - p. nzzc ‘L “A? “\ 9 I ‘ ‘1 u u , I. I“ ‘kv h ‘\" ~ 5 of capabilities than patronage had ever been capable of.7 Although technology's capacity for mass production of materials, with its correspondingly increased level of influence upon readers, made the content of mass media an issue of concern at a later stage in the debate over mass media, the primary criticism was first leveled at the society itself--the masses of society had no "taste."8 Though criticism of the mass society never really lessened, an influential critical blow was struck in the early thirties by Q.D. Leavis's Fiction and the Reading Public. Ms. Leavis criticized the mass society, the quality of the mass media and the level of morality she felt it contained. Her primary concern, however, was always clear. As a result of a long progressive process, beginning with the invention of printing, Leavis argued that "the general public--Dr. Johnson's common reader-- has now not even a glimpse of the living interests of modern literature, is ignorant of its grOwth and so prevented from developing with it, and the critical minority to whose sole charge modern literature has now fallen is isolated, disowned by the general public and threatened with extinction."9 In other words, the general public is so ignorant in matters of taste that it is about to commit the ultimate atrocity--to turn a deaf ear to the critics. In addition to her distrust of the general reading public, Leavis also blamed technology and commercialism ‘1' . in 5’ Q.’ .v;:." D ._. ..~-‘-..< ' U - I... —..- - —- O" h 'Ov". 0.-.»-U . --on . --., .. v ..,-. a... ._ _ O 0:9. :.,,' ' "' “0.-.. . c o ¢~... .-.- . . s ’ ‘ -~---.--: QC. 0-,. ‘.~'.‘- 5.... .....:;=. a. c ':."O a. .- -0.-.. ' ‘ O a ‘V ~ 9....-- ‘ ’- ......Da :- .4 ' ~ 0.. ‘ V b ‘ ‘cu- -‘ “‘ ¢.C,:.' .- . \ . . :I.’- “.. k - . - U- I o . .“ fi O . o... i. . a .u..: ‘zvu. I: o, o~.. “ “:5? t: . . Qua J ; ll" for creating an artistically inferior type of literature. Cheap novels, she believed (the paperback was just coming into prominence in England in the 1930's) provided the temptation for novelists to "specialize" that earlier novelists were never subjected to. As commercializa— tion increased, Leavis believed that novelists aimed directly to saturate the book market with rote, formula fiction aimed at the lower classes. By contrast, "it could never occur to an 18th century novelist to write to 10 In the 1920's and any but his peers," she claims. 1930's, this phenomenon, according to Leavis, turned the term "best seller" into "an almost entirely derogatory epithet among the cultivated."11 Recall that it was Leavis who coined the terms "highbrow, middlebrow and lowbrow" as suitable for use in describing the fiction of her period. The terms referred to the quality of the reader, the artistic com— petency of the writer and the level of morality embodied in the novels. About the latter, Ms. Leavis is empha- tically critical. She criticized lowbrow fiction for provoking "warm, vague, emotional feelings" which she says are associated with religion and religion substitutes. Key words in the lowbrow emotional vocabulary include life, death, love, good, evil, sin, home, mother, noble, gallant, purity, honor and so forth. As far as Leavis is concerned, these are "stock emotional responses" | . .. V'I .. ;I¢H'I I . .... ‘0'. . I u . . .vz-o - g.” ~ u...“ 5... -.... .Ov. .. : ; . Jib... n. I . i H". n. _ U 4 b- ‘gosf In.‘§ ‘ o -.;‘;o. ' ' Dlhowu. .‘.. . o :0... . a; '“‘ b..._‘ ‘ .. . .. -“ . - r—: r”. a. n.ng'.\_‘ . :o;--:"- - can‘t-A“). . l . .S‘v ‘ n. r J: ~.E ~. . A ‘-.‘ .~ ~.' Ii. :‘5 .V ;l ‘1- . ‘ . n; O I“.. out..' ..'.‘ O . O. ..‘ W ..’E . V n 5‘. ‘ A b. ‘. _ c-¢ C “. t: 1.. . t: ‘~:5. u.., ‘ Hosea..“‘ ‘§ 4 . . “-C' ‘ ~. kn.:~ 0.; . _ U ‘I- ‘ . «.560 ‘1 an: in .. .yvfi‘. ‘ fh.,:~..-_ ‘ "VHS 1 . .0 Q .'~ . »¥.§;“ .‘ - up. n .u A n‘ 'D “.“e ‘.'_ ‘4' E O 53 o o ‘ S;'€.v.‘h r'fi.‘ r C a I! .- ‘u:APq‘ ~ vt“.“ I .2_ ' ‘:.‘n C :‘q 0 t, ‘ . ." ,u which "every self-aware person finds that he has to train himself ... in withstanding."12 Writing forty years later, Edward Shils followed Leavis in attempting to divide artistic products into wide, value-laden categories. Shils writes, "For present purposes, we shall employ a very rough distinction among three levels of culture, which are levels of qual- ity measured by aesthetic, intellectual, and moral standards. These are 'superior' or 'refined' culture, 'mediocre' culture, and 'brutal' culture."13 In an attempt to avoid condemnation of the mass society itself, however, he stresses that the levels refer only to the works and not to the consumers or authors of those works. Like Leavis, he describes "superior" culture in terms of established literary analysis: it “is distinguished by the seriousness of its subject matter, that is, the centrality of the problems with which it deals, the acute penetration and coherence of its perceptions and the subtlety and wealth of its expressed feeling." "Mediocre” culture is that body of works which, "whatever the aspiration of their creators, do not measure up to the standards employed in judging works of superior culture." Brutal culture is characterized, according to Shils, by the absence of symbolic content, depth of penetration, subtlety, and by the presence of . . . . . 4 'a general grossness of sens1t1v1ty and perception."1 ——_’ lO Oddly enough, Shils fancies himself a defender of the legitimacy of mediocre and brutal culture. In championing his defense, however, Shils must resort to a discussion of the creators of and the audience of mediocre and brutal culture, which he precisely set out to avoid. In Shils' opinion, the redeeming virtue of "mediocre" culture is that "it is often earnestly, even if simply, moral." Shils goes on to say that works of mediocre culture "express something essential in human life, and expunging them would expunge the accumulated wisdom of ordinary men and women, their painfully developed art of coping with the miseries of existence, their routine 15 In other words, pieties and their decent pleasures." mediocre culture can be tolerated because it provides an outlet for the expression of the moral feelings of its authors and consumers. Though Shils does not identify the moral values that works of mediocre culture contain, it is plain that he considers them "good" values-- perhaps those that contribute to the maintenance of the status quo. It is also clear that he considers overt morality in literature "simple," however, and incompatible with aesthetic excellence. Shils is harder pressed to defend brutal culture, stating that it would be "frivolous to deny the aesthetic, moral and intellectual unsatisfactoriness of much of popular culture or to claim that it shows the human race . ...—n. -‘ O: 0“ . gun..- bud. ' ~v‘§-. o_-\— . 0 h . :o-cnv :-:¢... '0 '0 5. m.. .. .5 ‘. .- A n A. . “dos- ;-3 I i... . “-~.I . . 6v-~.‘ ‘ ‘ o‘n Q ‘ r’u- I . .“v 4" s ‘§‘-. g ' . C“; , -“‘u C. Q n l'” e ~~ .‘.:-"S :' I .2" u a .“'I ‘ ~_‘l‘:t:“: ~‘1 3'- 34.. .5 . :.'~- “I v 8“ ~~" ’ . t¥“ .‘ v V; .. H gin “‘ b; 5‘ ‘ a”: -h . I h. l\.::"'; ~ . s“b~ ... “ O ‘. \. .‘q ‘H‘: A. ' Q-‘bi _ C-, ‘u' m_ ~ 4‘ ". 4» .. S Q ~ ‘ 11 in its best light.“ Nonetheless, he finds a saving grace; popular culture used to be much more brutal even than it is today: The culture of these strata [the brutal culture], which were dulled by labor, illness and fear, and which comprised a far larger proportion of the population than they do in advanced societies in the twentieth cen- tury, was a culture of bear-baiting, cock— fighting, drunkenness, tales of witches, gossip about the sexual malpractices of priests, monks, and nuns, stories of murders and mutilations.l In other words, the good news is that the brutal masses are diminishing in numbers and progressing, though Shils doesn't indicate where the progression is leading. A recent article by Raymond Williams, "On High and Popular Culture," furnishes some perspective on the concept of mass society and its cultural products. Williams argues that high culture represents the best art, practices, and products from every culture in every age. He warns, however, that this high culture is dominated and controlled--in effect identified--in any given period by the dominant culture and by the particular institutions of that culture that are charged with preserving the high culture. He writes: Thus high culture, the work of more than one's own class, society, period or even epoch, is commonly incorporated into a particular contemporary social structure-- a social class or such institutions as universities or churches--that owes its 12 real contemporary existence to factors other than high culture, and that indeed often confuses its temporary, local or self- interested features with the received and selected high culture that it offers to justify or to ratify them. 17 Williams argues that such a restrictive view of what constitutes "high culture" inevitably leads to the masking of the real meanings and values contained within the art forms. "Abstract and pseudo-universal definitions of high culture and popular culture," he warns, "lead us to evade true cultural values and contemporary reality."18 In stating the need to recognize many centers of meaning and value, Williams is arguing not only for the legitimacy of serious popular culture study, but is also urging that critics of both high and popular culture attempt to minimize their cultural biases. It becomes apparent, then, that the criticism of the mass society contains both an implicit criticism of the values of the non-elite classes as well as a criticism of the mass media products they prefer. Bell notes that a central tenet of the mass society criticism is fear of a "general breakdown of values," and Leavis and Shils illustrate this fear by their discussions of the "lowbrow" or "brutal" culture. In addition, however, the critics of mass society view mass media products not only as morally dangerous but as artistically inferior, just as the technology that made mass printing possible was n . . . .. sup n';. ’. . ' - o.t..ohi ‘ . . ..... .: . Ar__ .. 3...... IDQI'V.A':..- on...,'.‘_-“ I ‘NA' ....O ..~,. . t . ;.. 0: ~--_.. a... "“~ \ _ ..., _ . I- - . F- .-. .00. '. do. w . . 1:...~ -5. .. .uu..u‘ F“. u ‘ a .u.. “- . I“ .1 r... §n~o.‘~ ‘--. c '.':.:‘Q "‘a s'scby .‘..9 C... . _~v. 'u.. ~ ' n-.. V- ‘0‘.‘ ..bu . :3: s ‘_ | 5 .§‘..=.“. \ “H. ' - .~ ‘ " 3s... ““ .‘y‘. ~l ‘v. ‘§ . “‘\‘:~. F ‘ ‘.~:‘ '1 3. ~‘ ‘ Q‘ ‘ ..' h — I‘ n A .‘ “t: res \ . ‘. ". .Q o 3~ ,; ~~ " 99“ U‘ .A . n“ ..V N 1". U V ‘ " c.~ -" -‘ ‘ 5.. ~\~’. | ‘~ ‘ “ ‘ \"\: ~. ‘. .... - .\ ... “.. A, - ~~ . “v- \. -‘ ‘A ”s F,‘ ‘1“ . \\~ S ~ . “:S . I q \“ ‘ h.‘:;. ‘. ‘ ‘ ‘\ .C ‘I Q \~\ ‘l‘ufi ct “ s r- ~_ ‘ - s s 13 deplored by eighteenth century elite critics. Leavis eschews formula fiction; film is only recently being incorporated into academic curricula; broadcasting is still rigorously excluded in all but "popular culture" and "audio-visual aids" departments. In Shils' definition of superior and mediocre culture, no broad- casting media are included, though mediocre culture can include "relatively novel genres not yet fully incor- porated into superior culture," such as the musical comedy, Shils says.19 The modern-day prophet of the mass media, Marshall McLuhan, is concerned to locate the values that are transmitted by the technological form of a given 20 He argues that the most influential medium itself. transmission of values takes place, not through content at all, but through the very form of the media. That is, "The medium is the message," as his famous phrase goes. According to McLuhan, there is a very different set of values associated with print and electric tech- nology, each of which is related to the forms of the media themselves. Furthermore, McLuhan believes that the values associated with technological form cannot be understood through content analysis. "The effects of technology do not occur at the level of opinions or concepts," he writes, "but alter sense ratios or patterns of perception 21 steadily and without any resistance." McLuhan argues that although the values associated with print technology a a .u .. .- "' Ila.- .. ion-lg .. - a. '_ I 5" , uuu..-§. ”h o 00:.- ". ‘ ‘ 0‘ ‘ . s«._’: . b I I...- | « _ “A-"..~ .' Ah-y...._..' -. . ' '5' L 5...: o..""u.\ V ‘ ‘a-r... .' ""-..l .... ‘- . ‘ ‘ x . OA- ‘j ‘- V;~-~_ . .. I -. . .- " .“ u. - “'"-". II o H.‘ _ ,. .:: ~.“' . i. . I "t A ... I. Q “~ V-- 5 g ‘( ‘ a \‘ ‘K “ ‘ V ~ -‘ ..h ‘ b“ “1‘ 'U v l v- ‘ !‘ C n a - qtl‘g' I: A- \- \ r. . q “ \ “qs_- 1‘_‘: \ ~ 14 are taken for granted in the United States, that an outsider (he notes that he is Canadian) can understand these values and see how they work in the culture. Accordingly, McLuhan thinks Alexis de Tocqueville‘s perceptive comments about American life in the nineteenth century grew out of his ability to detach himself from the predominant American media form. McLuhan writes, "De Tocqueville was a highly literate aristocrat who was quite able to be detached from the values and assumptions of typography. And it is only on those terms, standing aside from any structure or medium, that its principles and lines of force can be discerned."22 As an example of McLuhan's position, he argues that the phonetic alphabet, alone, was responsible for the creation of civilization as we know it. "Separateness of the individual, continuity of space and of time, and uniformity of codes are the prime marks of literate and O I O O I 0 2 ClVlllzed soc1eties," he writes. 3 Simply because of its straight line formation on the page, print accustomed individuals to thinking in terms of a linear perspective and allowed us to develop the concept of point of view, claims McLuhan.24 Literacy, which has prompted uniformity as a value, has, in turn, per McLuhan, been responsible for the development of the producer-consumer system in the United States. It is a characteristic of both goods and consumers O {... .. .. . I. . ‘3 vv- ‘O h u .. ‘ .1? l'. v‘ .. - L...” . . ‘: «...... ' o 'Io-g . I IIUCv“. ..-vl . — . ... .“".vu v I . ~ ..A ~ ’ . ‘. M Il.‘..~.'. ' a . ‘ u =.. “.-“v-. . v. ..‘ .... :O . '-..‘_ .I‘ u. . ‘l "h‘ ..I~ .-. .v .4 ._.~. I. ‘v . ‘a ‘-~.' M. w ...» §¢..~‘. A. A ...: E ‘9 s‘ 5" .l . ="n { “ CW‘ “-..- ~‘~- .. N“ ~C’s s-..‘ . N \“‘ C.‘9- a “Q. \ " :. c.‘~' '0‘ ‘. ‘s 1.: h .‘ . ‘ H “\.‘~-l :‘ ‘— “a I. ' ~ . "I: Q ‘ : ‘\ s. :I; . ‘- a ‘ ..‘nn‘ ‘ ‘5 ‘1 a ”s 5“ ‘ C‘O..c‘fi .‘ 5‘ ‘ ‘s “s: ..h' v. . “‘ \v“‘ u 9 s ‘ - ¥. \‘6; .“ ‘ «h. v‘.. \ . u . \v. \ ~. \“-- « 15 in this country that they are standardized in appearance and quality (or beliefs).25 From the first, McLuhan argues, America incorporated print technology into its educational, industrial and political life and the formula proved very rewarding financially because uniform, stan- dardized work performance is highly efficient and highly productive, hence highly profitable.26 Thus, uniformity and standardization are two values which McLuhan associ— ates with the visual print culture.27 Tolerance is another value which McLuhan traces to the linear perspective of the eye: The ear is hyperesthetic compared to the neutral eye. The ear is intolerant, closed, and exclusive, whereas the eye is open, neutral, and associative. Ideas of tolerance came to the West only after two or three centuries of literacy and visual Gutenberg culture. No such saturation with visual values had occurred in Germany by 1930. Russia is still far from any such involvement with visual order and values.2 Likewise, McLuhan asserts that respectability, defined as "the ability to sustain visual inspection of one's life," did not become dominant until after printing, and that it still remains unimportant in non-visually oriented Europe.29 The analogy he uses to illustrate his point is the difference in general appearance between American women and European women. He claims that American women have never been equaled in any culture ‘A' .qv~ a. nu. w.... !.3..:‘. q '.v “o C in... . ‘ Q t .‘b e o. . I v ‘, .‘A. a t... “.'J:' a a-.. . "H‘e 5:- «...: '1 . 4 no 1- 1‘ AU #4 ! fi 09") (V ‘11 16 for "visual turnout," (by which I presume he means visual attractiveness), but that to Europeans, women of our culture look like "abstract, mechanical dolls." The appearance of EurOpeans and of the EurOpean coun- tryside, by contrast, "has always been shoddy in American eyes," says McLuhan. In related fashion, asexuality during an enforced and prolonged adolescence has been a visual value of Americans. This is a result of the printed media stress (which is compatible with middle class emphasis on education, training, and accumulation of wealth) on taking the long, straight look toward the future and sacrificing immediate pleasure for long term goals. McLuhan says that in Europe there is the direct passage from childhood to adulthood, and that since television, American adoles- cence has similarly disappeared.30 Although McLuhan is aware that the visual values of print technology are still important in American culture, he argues that they can not be pro- longed indefinitely. He claims that this is not a "question of values," or a matter which can be dealt with by rational consideration and reasoned preference: If we understood our older media, such as roads and the written word, and if we valued their human effects sufficiently, we could reduce or even eliminate the electronic factor from our lives. Is there an instance of any culture that understood the technology that sustained its structure and was prepared T.“"" I". 0"- 9‘- in. F A... --_. .t. .. .9- .\q A;- 5‘. .i ...-u, ._ " I “‘ who‘s. ' \ 'I... .. "\ I ‘ ~“.Uon..‘ 5.; ‘s "' ... “h. .....‘"Ova '0 .I u... ave v- . Q ‘ 17 to keep it that way? If so, that would be an instance of values or reasoned preference. The values or preferences that arise from the mere automatic operation of this or that technology in our social lives are not capable of being perpetuated.31 In other words, since we don't understand (or even understand the importance of understanding) the technology of our media forms, we cannot act in such a way as to preserve the values they embody. What then are the values that McLuhan asso- ciates with the new electronic media? Here McLuhan is less certain, except insofar as he can tell us what they are not. McLuhan considers that television is the electronic medium which has been responsible for a progressive disappearance of all "lineality in living." Gone, he says, are the staff and line struc- tures in management, the stag line, the party line, the receiving line and "the pencil line from the backs 32 of nylons." Electricity, argues McLuhan, is "instant information,“ and the electric medium of television demands the involvement of all the senses, as opposed to the visually-oriented print media. Thus, a decline in all of the values associated with the neutral eye can be expected.33 McLuhan sees evidence of this in the move away from standardization in consumer goods and the movement toward non-conformity in Q o - oo..., Viv—4....I o' . . V. -~-~. d . IO'-~.“‘. e'. : .'R~ , Q ‘1‘ N» u “\ ..V'V~ t, '0‘ A 2‘.. - .. -..\ A. ..."‘V .. ‘- .:- .‘ “5 Ce ‘ I I’A b iIJv' OU_. b . - b ‘ b. “‘ ‘ \ s‘h‘e JJ . I. c I ‘. A; ~ ‘5 a \‘ “~"‘ u. "s 4' U. n" I N ‘u. ..‘k 3.- . § \.‘ ‘ t V 6.. s‘:.“ ‘s~b,l. 5‘. . ‘x ‘n! 5F ‘\Q'v‘ .. g V '1. U h ‘\ a ac~K ‘.“‘~ 9 18 clothing, hair styles, transportation, living styles, eating habits and so forth. Since electricity is instant information, which can be transmitted virtually as fast as it is created, electricity gives primacy to "process," says McLuhan.34 By process, McLuhan means to include both the process of creating media content and the process of experiencing it. But, in addition, he implies that attention to process is in itself an electric value. In fact, the term "mass media" takes on new meaning when one considers the effects of the electronic media, argues McLuhan. "In entertainment media," he says, "we speak of this fact as 'mass media' because the source of the program and the process of experiencing it are independent in space, yet simultaneous in time." Thus the term “mass media" comes to indicate not mass production, which is a characteristic of the print technology, but "instant simultaneous experience," since everybody becomes involved in it at the same time.35 Though he grants television much dignity in his analysis, and considers it in no way second class, McLuhan is obviously concerned about the effect that electronic values will have on our culture. Since television requires the intense participation of all our senses, he calls it a "tactile" medium, the sense . I O .0 o-~-~u P U. Dvdu§0 ‘ | Cro:0. a. p . doood-cvoa ~ mm... , »~.:|:. , :0}. ‘ a p V $ \. ‘5'. s a“ I P.‘ .‘2‘Cu 8‘ t “K - 19 of touch being most closely correlated with the coor- dination of overall sensory activity. He goes on, however, to pose a frightening analogy between elec- tronic technology and the human central nervous system. Via this analogy, McLuhan tries to show the meaning— lessness of content analysis of electric media. He states: Electric technology is directly related to our central nervous systems, so it is ridic- ulous to talk of 'what the public wants' played over its own nerves. Once we have surrendered our senses and nervous systems to the private manipulation of those who would try to benefit from taking a lease on our eyes and ears and nerves, we don't really have any rights left. Leasing our eyes and ears and nerves to commercial interests is like handing over the common speech to a private corporation, or like giving the earth's atmosphere to a company as a monopoly. McLuhan believes that electronic media forms, especially television, because of the total and inclu- sive involvement that they demand, are creating the need for intensive social involvement, external con- sensus (or personal concordance) and a docile and meditative approach to life among the viewing public.37 In other words, McLuhan thinks that the print culture emphasis on long-term goals is no longer considered important by children of the electronic age, and that a countervailing emphasis on getting along in the present and on understanding one's own feelings and p A. U. ' .- on ‘e .4" -‘l 3 .w» an aid .u.‘ u .5 a. an v. o~ 3 ... D ...: ... 'I- 5‘.-- a n... 8“. I . . . a . a v :‘ a . . . . as P a a H 3 . . Pt o Hub» “no. .3 A: .0 a a: I . “u 20 those of others has arisen. This, claims McLuhan, is a result solely of the media form, and would be the same no matter what the content consisted of.38 The previous discussion has shown three major ways in which critics have linked the transmission of values to the mass media: the first assumes that the inferior taste and morality of the mass citizenry demands media products of corresponding inferiority; the second directly attacks mass media products (such as formula fiction, movies and broadcasting) as being "specialized" and "commercial" rather than artistic. Finally, McLuhan's attempt to find cultural values in the technological forms of the media represents a significant modification and elaboration of other broad generic criticism of mass media products. In pointing out the importance of media form, McLuhan raises a challenge to subsequent media analysis. Any attempt to consider values in media content must also attempt to deal with the relationship between media content and the values transmitted by the media form. So we come to the central question of this chapter--what about analysis of values in media con- tent? Is such analysis legitimate and possible? Critics in general would probably argue "yes," though it is clear that such an analysis must take into u ~nq,‘ --t Or . Ivvvfln. .... . . . u ~I4-n than F bego-‘ua ~ a ......mi . ‘fiv. A‘ " ...- q. .- . ' ......‘ . u “ Q; t "“.. : a t) v n I (.l ' H .' :v... I:.. “y ..aL',‘e :4 7. V C a? 31 () '01“ n 'f’ q A I . e ¥~“\:‘ VA -b.~v 3‘ '~ c “‘v “&. ‘ 'Q a“ Q I I “ 0:..- »... h ‘--.\ c! . 21 account the media form, and that one must avoid being blinded by one's own cultural mores. Leo Lowenthal, an early critic of p0pular culture, writes, "By studying the organization, content and linguistic symbols of the mass media, we learn about the typical forms of behavior, attitudes, commonly held beliefs, prejudices, and aspirations of large numbers of people."39 Russel Nye, author of the most compre— hensive text on the popular arts in America, affirms that values are transmitted by both pOpular and elite culture, though he claims they are different in kind: Popular art confirms the experience of the majority, in contrast to elite art, which tends to explore the new ... The popular artist corroborates (occasionally with great skill and intensity) values and attitudes already familiar to his audience; his aim is less to provide a 28w experience than to validate an older one. Richard Hoggart, fellow of the Center for Contemporary Cultural Studies at London's University of Birmingham, thinks that "works of literature at all levels are shot through with--irradiated with-- values, with values ordered and values acted out."41 He further believes that it is legitimate to suspend judgment about the aesthetic quality of the work and concentrate on values internal to the work. 3;: (I) f! 22 So, one isn't at this point specifically asking questions about the value of the work, but is trying to understand better what Weber called 'the relationship of the object to values.‘ One is trying to find, from as inter- nal a reading as possible, what this kind of work tells you about its society, about what that figciety believes, about its self-iden- tity. Hoqgart believes that "by creating orders within itself, art helps to reveal the orders of values present within a culture, either by mirroring or by resisting them and preposing new orders."43 For example, Ian Watt was able to discover, in his study of the birth of the English novel, that the social transition from a patriarchal to an indi- vidualist social and economic order was accompanied by a crisis in marriage which bore particularly hard on females. The fiction of the period reflected the increased value placed on marriage and reflected its increasing difficulty for females to obtain.44 Within a single novel such as Clarissa, more isolated values might emerge, values that only make sense when viewed in connection with other values in the novel and in other novels of the period. Clarissa valued her virginity and the approval of her parents; she also valued her freedom of choice and independence of action. Watt's analysis of changing marriage customs helps explain Clarissa's seemingly contradictory values, and I - .L- aunt a; ‘ -.= .9‘V.U&- 73.35. 'J . 5...... .., s.“:.‘ -s ... . ... H. “Ar. ':oocs r- V.. . n A. ‘L Fp.. ' "' at. Q A... v u d Qua m. V‘- § 'y“ - QA-. . "3:95 of 33" 53-95 1 3“'.‘9~ A C . C ‘0- V‘ I . nu' "... u ‘3 a VA ‘ ‘ ‘LA “‘t 3:;- . - .— A. 5 . '3 s‘« q.‘ “n “G-$ ' . ‘ ‘H5 H‘ ’. ‘1‘. ‘I b “‘ eh! 23 the choices she made based on her ordering of those values. Richard Hoggart says that the aim of literary students who have a special interest in understanding their culture "is to find eventually what field of values is embodied, reflected or resisted, within the work.”45 I interpret Hoggart to mean that the various values portrayed by a work must be analyzed in terms of the preferences that characters place on them. In other words, given a choice between two attitudes or courses of action, which does a given character choose? How does the sum of these choices stack up into a system of preferences? In essence, this is the method of values analysis employed by Lawrence Kohlberg, whose theory of values deve10pment has been chosen for use in the present study. In conclusion, then, this chapter has attempted to show that the relationship between human values and mass media products has been an important critical issue since the invention of printing and the subse- quent birth of the pOpular novel. It has further intended to demonstrate that the most fruitful approach to identifying this relationship lies in a culturally unbiased analysis of values in mass media content; and which also attempts to deal with the values con- veyed by the media form. Although the relationship ‘ )2." fill“- lu 1“ (‘l a '0’ .'“" G‘s. . :'v\g. .-- I... I bu‘. ~A..'- \ ii...“ a ‘~~. ‘ 'O‘..~‘ ‘Fn“-~ ‘ b ‘ , .§~' ..3 .. A D". .‘ '-~ IE :- ca... 3"“ 24 between media values and those of the viewing public is important to establish, criticism of the values of mass society is bound to degenerate into elitist generalities unless conducted by those trained in the analytical tools of social research. As Richard Hoggart points out, sweeping generic studies of pOpular culture arrive too quickly and insubstantially at bold generalizations. "Bad literature or mass literature or process literature may be as corrupting as we often say. But we have not made the claim convincing even to SYmpathetic outsiders ..."46 As will become more apparent as this study proceeds, the theory of values development developed by Lawrence Kohlberg offers the benefits of a philOSOphi- cally sound, practical, and culturally unbiased approach to the analysis of human values. Since Kohlberg's approach is structural as Opposed to strict- ly content-oriented, it offers the possibility of apprehending at greater depth the meaning which lies beneath judgments and actions of media characters. In addition, the structural aspect of the theory provides suggestions for analysis of values inherent in the media form, which, of course, contributes to the overall transmission of the value message. Before proceeding to an explanation of the Kohlberg theory and 25 a discussion of its precise application to the present research, it is apprOpriate to delve more deeply into the question of what constitutes a "value." Also, from a philOSOphical and historical perspective, major competing approaches to the analysis of values need to be explored in greater detail. This will be the subject of Chapter 2. ‘4 -r' PH ‘ ' U’ 6 c. b... g'IARI .... . '0 ”Va ‘4 26 Notes to Chapter 1 1Figures quoted in English Journal (December 1972), 1248. 2Herbert J. Gans, POpular Culture and High Culture; An Analysis and Evaluation of Taste (New York, 1974), 4-7. 3Raymond Bauer and Alice Bauer, "America: Mass Society and Mass Media," Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 16 (1960), 4-5. 4David Bell, “The Theory of Mass Society," Commentary, XXII (1956), 75-83. 5 Bell, 75. 6Leo Lowenthal, P0pular Culture and Society (Englewood Cliffs, N. J., 1961), 74. 7Ian Watt, The Rise of the Novel: Studies in Defoe, Richardson and Fielding (Berkeley, 1957), 54. 8Lowenthal, 52-108. Lowenthal describes the process through which criticism of the popular novel came to center first on the taste of the readers, then on the lowbrow authors and most lately on the content of the novels and its potential effect on readers, particularly young girls. 90. D. Leavis, Fiction and the Reading Public (London, 1932), 35. 10 Ibid., 163. llIbid., 34. lzIbid., 64-75. 13Edward Shils, The Intellectuals and the Powers and Other Essays (Chicago, 1972), 232. l4Ibid., 232-233. 151bid., 234. 161bid., 261. 17RaYmond Williams, "On High and Popular Cul- ture.“ New Republic (November 23, 1974), 14. II F "lo. .5 , -- .VAI'. \ (I! (*1 ’ t’\ (‘J lBIbid., 19Shils, 20 27 16. 232. McLuhan's analysis of the meaning and impact of print versus electric technology is contained in Marshall McLuhan, of Man (New York, 21 221bid., 23Ibid., 24Ibid., 251bid., 261bid. 271bid., 281bid., 29Ibid., 3OIbidl, 311bid., 321bid., 331bid., 34Ibid., 351bid., 361bid., 37Ibid., 381bid., 39 Understanding Media: The Extensions McLuhan, 1964). 33. 30. 86-87. 141-142. 33; 85-86; 281. 197. 264. 283. 282. 93-94. 280. 108. 301. 301, 303. 73. 64-65. 225. Lowenthal, xii. 40 Russel Nye, The Unembarrassed Muse: The Egpplar Arts in America (New York, 1970), 4. u. v a 2“ ..S‘W" war" I‘ §._ . . -, ‘O n at. " b»v¢-.-_. :1. \ ‘ :5” ~_ .- .-.. 'bt .". l-‘ ...- J- J- 0" J- 28 41Richard Hoggart, Contemporary Cultural Studies: An Approach to the Study of Literature and Society (Center for Contemporary Cultural Studies, Birmingham University, March 1969), 19. 42 Ibid., 10. 43Ibid., 11. 44Watt, 148. 45Hoggart, 9. 461bid., 7. ‘ £0.31. 4 u A" ‘i‘w’l‘l‘ ‘ 71"” ..m . -II A. ' u. A. ‘. a. nu e..,e in “a ' ‘ s; . ‘t- .e‘ 5“. \ "‘h... a 4 I. U.- .» ‘l u ‘ I ‘;.“ ‘AI. Ian-A4. V‘.‘ o P.‘ V‘ 'w “c: t. ‘ i . \ Q“'~~ . y “‘2‘ CHAPTER 2 WHAT ARE VALUES? SOME PHILOSOPHICAL DEFINITIONS OF VALUE The previous chapter has shown that critics of elite and popular culture have been concerned about the relationship between values and the mass media. It goes without saying, however, that the concept of "value" is complex, and one which critics seldom attempt to define concisely. Since I chose a particular definition of values--Lawrence Kohlberg's--for use in this study, it seems apprOpriate to review briefly how values have been defined historically before discussing what I believe to be the superiority of Kohlberg's approach. Therefore, the purpose of this chapter will be to review the historical development of value theory. Before Kohlberg, attempts to define value took either a nomothetic or idiographic approach.1 In terms of value theory, the nomothetic approach is based on the belief that the locus of values is in the external world, and that values are transmitted to the human organism as always normative, obligatory, and legitimate. The nomothetic approach may be either absolutist (all values are the same for everyone, or at least everyone in a given culture) or relativist (values depend on unique experiences that happen to individuals), but in Either case, values are believed to come to the person 29 .\ \.\ s . . i a s «e I» . l 4 . ‘ “H. u 4 . . \w v \ ‘3 Ne Ms ‘qu u ». ~41 t ..suu a... 3. .... .1. I ‘ 2 . u. an ‘ a‘ .. . n. d: a. .. . ....4 . . .. k . 1 ..a ax u~ A.» Q.» 4 s a .. a 2. w «E .1 4: \r‘ w. a». w». S ..s w.- ... Q r ..r‘ & bu ‘ "Q ism! Y c ‘ §-\-~ .t... ;. ~:~ ‘3‘. C ‘ 00.- ‘ -.!:‘ ‘ ‘§ :x: ‘2‘ ..~~ ‘3-“v M““ 8...- N.S 1‘- ‘.c \N ‘- . X E . ... . - . .rm. o as “an e “in. “mew man“ .x . . .. n w. . f .. A. - .0“ u v .o _ o .4. a u u .... _ M " Mum 35w uu-M saw I u 3 .. a: m . . a n o :3 1 30 from society and culture. The idiographic approach, on the other hand, takes the Opposite view and postu- lates that values are a creation of the person's own mind. According to this approach, values are relative to the experience of each individual; therefore, it is not fair to judge them by any external or “objective" standard. In the past, these two approaches to values have tended to be mutually exclusive, and theorists in both camps have failed to arrive at practical means for analysis of either individual or cultural values. To better understand the mutually exclusive and limited nature of the idiographic and nomothetic approaches, this chapter will briefly discuss the theories gener- ated by major philosoPhers in each camp. The philosoph- ical overviews will be followed by a look at the most modern practitioners of each approach. Since the idiographic philosophers' claims regarding the prac- tical application of their theories for values analysis are modest in comparison to those of nomothetic theo- rists, the discussion will begin with them. Early prominent idiographic philosophers were concerned with personal values as they affected social justice. Jeremy Bentham (1748-1832) led a group of political humanitarians in England called the Philosoph- ical Radicals (James Mill, father of Stuart Mill: was . n tb'ouo‘b 'u .. .‘46. . . _ ‘ ‘IMO..u. n. . H. ‘2 ‘3‘ . 5“..‘ :h-‘v~ ‘v‘ A. tiny-.... . 1.. "“7. a. ‘v' . “ ~; o 5 I“... ‘ saw...‘. . ‘ IA... . I: -:~ . ...“ ’ A‘.:u ‘. "u. .‘i. ‘ I &~._ hsi:a=:_‘. “§ I o ‘ I 1‘; '4‘. . _‘ N. s‘ “m, 2 0" ‘0 ‘l ‘A M ~‘ \u.-e;“ ‘... l'. ‘1 l I "‘7‘. 3- fl‘ . ) IN~ l! ,_ .x. P.‘ V“ ‘~ s". A-n .‘N‘W-J ‘s..‘-_' V. N n ‘. § M 5‘. \:~. 31 among the members), whose focus was the encouragement of legislation favorable to poor peOple. In particular, however, Bentham's great influence on nineteenth century men and women of letters grew out of his doctrine of utilitarianism. In terms of value theory, Bentham's principle of the greatest good for the greatest number is called "universal teleology." Teleologists, as a subgroup of axiologists, or value theorists, are con- cerned with the comparative amount of good that any act will cause. In his belief that pleasure or satis- faction is the source and measure of the good, Bentham was in agreement with previous idiographic philosophers, including the sephists, hedonists and epicureans, all of whom advocated pleasure as the greatest good. Bentham, however, denied that personal good always equals universal good, believing instead that it is necessary for individuals and society as a system to act in such a way as to maximize the pleasure of the many. The great pOpularizer of value theory in nineteenth century EurOpe was Friedrich Nietzsche (1844-1900). His primary tenet was that values are man-made and only temporarily stable and useful. Accordingly, Nietzsche interpreted history as the continuous creation and annihilation of values. Christianity, to him, represented an outdated set of 32 values, and he proposed that a new human culture based on more realistic contemporary values should replace it. His analysis of the values of the edu- cated middle class as antiquated led many peOple to consciously seek more realistic ideals. Since Nietzsche was primarily concerned with evolving a philosophy of life, however, his theory was more concerned with prescribing values that seemed to work in contemporary society rather than analyzing a range of values or attempting to identify the most worthwhile values. As such, he advocated a self-centered, strong-willed approach to life which would be able to withstand the forces of political and economic competition that were in control of nineteenth century social structures.3 Though economic theories of value have been primarily nomothetic, some early twentieth century economists were consciously idiographic or subjective in orientation. Franz Brentano (1837-1917), Alexius Meinong (1853-1921) and Frieherr von Ehrenfels (1859-1932) found the locus of value in the motives (or intentions), pleasure and appetites, respectively, of the valuer. Along somewhat similar lines, the .American axiologist Ralph E. Perry (1876-1957) believed that value came about as a result of the interest taken in any given object by the valuer. Perry's theory was in part nomothetic, since he thought that ‘ A; .' , v“. 'Q' .... .‘ioout ... n .c -.: ~ "U. I. "‘ . .. N ,1 II .. Q --..1 \- o ...... “'1 (I!) III ..., n1; ' “ a ll .."‘ -..‘vPvu .~‘s u ‘ o .A- ‘ a U. ~ . :V~-' .I. 0- '4 t u 21"-. I‘ v‘ . ‘f'u . “‘..~ . v ‘ ‘5 a .. v. m‘ 1 ‘ 's ‘ s .. . ~“u" ‘p. 5 \n ‘n q: ‘.:‘ ‘. o s: V s. O I o c - :u... K. . ‘-.,v~r V ‘1“ 27‘ ‘- ‘- v. “:3 o.; ' \ s . '3”. ‘\:“L W ‘ I ‘ R Q“:‘ s ‘ t u ‘ T ~.. _ ‘ ~ I‘:; ‘ ‘\ s Q i 5; “:n‘v ’ ’ fi 9 .4 w‘. ‘4 .- \ .. ‘\ “ y. . \ sh“. ‘\ . . ‘u a. u“ ‘, - . \.“‘ ‘\ ‘Q i "Q ‘- ‘ N‘ “x \ c ‘ s‘ .Q‘ \~ ‘ \ ‘N i ‘r‘ — 33 value realistically resided in objects in the environ- ment. He qualified this, however, with his theory that value only comes into existence at the moment when interest is taken in such objects. For example, "the silence of the desert lacks value until the moment when the wandering traveler finds it desolate and terrifying; this is also true in the case of the waterfall until human sensitivity finds it sublime," writes Perry.4 Of course, the idiographic approach to values largely avoids the necessity of ranking values and making judgements as to the correctness of a given individual's preferences. If value is not intrinsic, it cannot be experienced the same by everyone, so it is unfair to judge the motives, appetites or interests of another, since each person's experiences, and thus values, will be legitimately different. This, in effect, was the position taken by British philosopher/ scientist Bertrand Russell. In his treatise on Religion and Science (1935), Russell stated that "the chief ground for adopting this view [subjectivist] is the complete impossibility of finding any arguments to prove that this or that has intrinsic value." Russell, however, was not content to leave the issue without further discussion. He acknowledged what he perceived as a “desire to be \ Im. a“. .' 'v’vl' ”~ 4 ‘ .0 .... “‘I u n \. .3... 3 ...“ . O 3‘31: .: u to.»a..g"." 0 .-.~ 3“ a V‘ ‘_ ,1 a: "l""I~u-~ I . :~. :4- ~«-. n." ‘5‘ ‘vx . 4'. _ _ 3 ‘. {EH-:4 J Int:. “ ‘ . ~4ku .. “V: \ ; . :3 Q :h‘. ‘u. cvovd' 5““ i. ‘ ‘ v“:n_ \ ... .\ ..ac,‘ .3: ......“ . 4Qb._e “I I u .3 . "' 3~“6 ‘- ‘.- ‘ ‘ P . 6 A L Q" b‘ 3‘45 5...! V H 0‘ A h‘ §‘v ‘.: 34 'good'" on the part of most people. This desire to be good, however, according to Russell, "generally resolves itself into a desire to be approved, or alternately, to act so as to bring about certain general consequences which we desire." Regardless, Russell accused some peOple of having "imprOper" or "petty" 5 As one critic of Russell's theories has desires. noted, if he truly believed there were no such thing as intrinsic value, he could not have made this accu- sation, since he could not be completely confident that his tastes or values were more prOper or less petty. Russell was plainly convinced of "the existence of justice, decency and dignity. . ." though he refused to admit it since he could not make a scientific case for it.6 More recently, some social scientists have attempted to measure the existence of subjective values. Rollo Handy, in his survey The Measurement of Values: Behavioral Science and Philosophical Approaches, presents L. L. Thurstone as typical of this group.7 Thurstone has conducted a number of studies of preferences dis- played by certain subjects for particular nationalities, moral values, consumer goods, menu items, handwriting styles and so on. Thurstone's primary measurement technique has been self-reporting, usually via survey- questionnaire. He does not measure behavior since he I?“ .5 YES . .-AVV“ - n... I“..‘~ - '0‘. ‘“3- “A- I§vsu 5v. ‘ l .u.- ‘~-‘ _ Cusv‘-l‘t‘ 1': .G ’ 1‘ \. ' \I no ‘ 3 a. . A \stuau‘J‘ . A ‘ “4:24! ~,” '34. I. I H L’ \. F. u“ ‘ 4 o ‘ ‘ L; H ‘H “" :V C“' s A , .‘\~ W ‘1. “' ‘1 ‘ I .. , 0‘ ‘1 f \ . oh‘ ‘4: ‘A:_' s. ‘u: ~_ )A ‘ “Q A.“ V-. 35 believes that values may not be reflected by behavior. He says: A man may be entirely consistent in what he says and in what he does about a controversial issue, and yet both of these indices may be dead wrong in reflecting his attitude . . . His personal attitudes may or may not agree with what he says and what he does. Here again, attitudes are essen— tially subjective experiences whicg may or may not conform with overt action. Although Thurstone argues that understanding and predictability Of certain values can be gained by his method, the problem Of how one ought to value, given conflicting social or moral claims, remains untouched by his approach. The unwillingness to attempt a ranking of values, or to acknowledge the superiority of some values over others is, in my Opinion, the major failing of other modern idiographic philoSOphers. These groups of philOSOphers, who usually speak of themselves as "relativists," can be divided into two broad groups. One group maintains that values are culturally relative, but that the values of (in par- ticular, this) culture are Obligatory on all who wish to remain a part of the society. Choosing one's culture (which, of course, almost nobody really does) becomes the means by which the free will selects a set of values. The other branch Of relativism, which has several \l .‘ ..A*n- q‘ ‘95 win-:y.‘ ":"-v- ...-«“3‘3' 1 \ I ‘A 3.. I In \ . '::'l‘u-‘ . a ‘ ‘vev ...-.... At 2“ 1. ‘. . ~ 1A”; I V O 59:... - ‘ n‘ ‘3: c- ~U 8“: \‘~ h‘ M \ \ 4 2“. .1 / (I 36 self-conscious subgroups--including existentialists, humanists, and values-clarificationists--believe that all values are relative. Every individual is judged responsible for the formation of his or her own values, and it is considered unfair for another individual, who has not had the same set Of experiences and cannot be in possession Of the same knowledge and perspective, to pass judgment on those values.9 The most clearly defined relativistic values movement in the United States today is the values clarification movement. Louis E. Raths, its founder, and his principal disciple, Sidney Simon, now considered the movement‘s leader, are humanists with a concern for what they perceive as the lack Of values among school children. In their classic textbook on values- clarification, Values and Teaching: Working with Values in the Classroom, Raths, Simon and co—author Merrill Harmin, say that “too many children in the schools today . . . do not seem to learn as well as they might Ibecause they simply are not clear about what their lives are for, what is worth working for."10 Implicit in the procedure of the clarificationists is the idea that 22x_purpose is better than no purpose at all, certainly in terms of motivating students to learn in the class- room . ' .4. .4 at..l|. ‘-Q I ';..bu- and o--- :- ~ua . V ”V out-- ...,v.. 0-... .. '. ...... I . V ..‘t a ' o o ..' I not a . U. ' . . fi‘I-u _- ‘ l \ _V‘.~ ""“"~‘a.. I ' 1 P a “. ‘. ,'_. ‘. 37 In their attempt to focus on the process of valuing and to provide practical guidance for the teacher, Raths, Simon and Harmin focus on seven selec- tion criteria which they posit as necessary for some- thing to be considered a value. These are: choosing l) freely, 2) from alternatives, 3) after thoughtful consideration of the consequences of each alternative; and 4) cherishing, being happy with the choice;; 5) being willing to affirm the choice publicly; 6) action, or doing something with the choice; and 7) making the choice a part of some pattern of life. The authors also identify eight indicators of values, xflhich they say are like values in many respects, but ‘Mhich do not possess all of the above seven criteria. 'rhese are: goals or purposes; aspirations; attitudes; .interests; feelings; beliefs and convictions; activities; iind worries, problems, or obstacles.11 The problem with the values-clarification approach, however, is revealed in the following inter— cflnange between a teacher and her students, used as an «example of the technique in Raths, Simon and Harmin. The teacher has asked the class for possible alternative WaYS to view honesty. Teacher: . . . Well, then, let's list the four possibilities that we have on the board and I'm going to ask that each of you do two things for yourself: (1) see if you can identify any other choices in this issue of 544‘ n\~ .«u. 38 honesty and dishonesty, and (2) consider the consequences of each alternative and see which ones you prefer. Later, we will have buzz groups in which you can discuss this and see if you are able to make a choice and if you want to make your choice part of your actual behavior. This is something you must do for yourself. Ginger: Does this mean that we can decide for ourselves whether we should be honest on tests here? Teacher: No, that means that you can decide on the value. I personally value honesty; and although you may choose to be dishonest, I shall insist that we be honest on our tests here. In other areas Of your life, you may have more freedom to be dishonest, but one can't do anything any time, and in this class I shall expect honesty on tests. Ginger: But then how can we decide for ourselves? Aren't you telling us what to value? Sam: Sure, you're telling us what we should do and believe in. Teacher: Not exactly. I don't mean to tell you what you should value. That's up to you. But I do mean that in this class, not else- where necessarily, you have to be honest on tests or suffer certain consequences. I merely mean that I cannot give tests without the rule of honesty. All Of you who choose dishonesty as a value may not practice it here, that's all I'm saying. Further questions anyone?12 It is obvious here that the teacher is talking out; of both sides of her mouth. She has set up a sitluation whereby students "choosing" dishonesty freely, from alternatives, after thoughtful consideration <3 f"the consequences of each alternative, and happily, 1e child not then go on to affirm the choice publicly, act 39 it out or incorporate it into a life pattern ("you can cheat elsewhere, but not here"). Had the students really been alert, a logical outgrowth Of such an inter- change might have been to question the value of testing, or any other means of examination that would require the "rule" of honesty. In other words, it is clear that the teacher values both testing and honesty and that she considers not valuing them, invalid. Under the Kohlberg method of teaching values development, it would be the teacher's role to discover the stage level of those students valuing dishonesty, and by answering their arguments with logical consistency at the next higher stage, impress them with the superior logic and morality of honesty. Surely this is a more "honest" approach for the teacher who values honesty to take. In marked contrast to idiographic theorists, nOmothetic philosophy has been full of prescriptive judgments regarding how people gught to value and which values are higher than others. Recall that the nomo- thetic view contends that values are contained a priori in"the environment, merely waiting to be apprehended. AS Such, philosophers and social scientists of this per:Suasion have believed it is possible to identify ax“3 occasionally rank values by deductive reasoning rld/Or some type of Objective measurement. The1r ; u ‘. basai '1 ‘ i-aiib' n. l .- ~v-w~ :-3 MQDQVO' 9 .... wt.) . l‘ll “a," u I I 4.!“ u- ‘1..‘ a v.“ "‘-05u . Uh.” Oh. ut.._._ ...: "~L. . hi 4'~;~ . Viol..‘.’ . ' . ....‘- ..‘ ON. ."‘ :va'av q Q bl...‘. . .Q . u.“ "i e ..H sA-~_..‘ q -~ I a ‘u. \.“. .... - ‘ un' - , ‘1 fig:: - "4E:S ‘ \ I“. ‘u‘ s'.' ‘5. s :E".I‘ \“ F \- “'4‘. 5.. .~. : ‘ ‘ t ~_ . ‘ #:le l ‘u l \ \ 1'- 40 methods, however, have been tenuous and subject to criticisms of various sorts, and their results have been very limited in scope. The first discipline to deal overtly with theories of value was economics. Early economists were nomothetic, defining value as that which is contained within goods which renders them valuable. During the ancient and medieval periods, goods were exchanged directly for other goods and for services, with little use being made of money as a symbolic carrier Of value. Later, in the mercantilist period (fifteenth and sixteenth centuries), money was consid- ered valuable in its own right. Growing out of the belief Of the time that one nation's gain, in terms of wealth, was of necessity another's loss, economists believed it was crucial to sell more goods and services than were bought, in order to stockpile bullion. Bullion, in turn, was used to enrich the personal Coffers of the sovereign and to purchase military forces arki supplies to protect and enlarge the sovereign's territory. Such economic value theories were said to sQQk a "favorable balance of trade."13 John Locke (1632-1704), David Hume (1711-1776), David Ricardo (1772-1823), and Karl Marx (1818-1883) were all“Ong the best known economists to talk in terms Of F‘ufl‘fl I n A- . -' l . sfidb| v. 1173.18. O~' .6‘a4‘ ' i ‘ n 9‘ ti; 0,. .. "' “It 0“.. \_‘ "o- ~§~ . . 'OAA .— O-D~. ‘ 3“! ‘N “x“ w \s‘ .‘. ‘Q ‘ U-»§‘c. “t: . ... ‘ Va "we “1 I so. ‘3 a . _ ~ ‘.‘.H Uu.‘ -“' ... . :':' ‘§ " ifi. ‘ W‘s . § §. 3 . a ‘5'.— . w.“ u .- ‘ Q N ' “‘ ‘ i > a "6. ~ . "\‘ tv: I A“ . d"‘:‘ a- a I ‘D . . r"!~'~'~. a “:3. AA p .‘ .J.¢ :1" v‘H s " ... . '5 ‘-..,‘- ... . 41 labor, as opposed to goods, as the primary source of value. These men varied greatly, however, regarding what claims they believed the laborer had to the fruits of his labor.14 Beginning with Adam Smith (1723-1790), human nature itself began to be viewed as a source of economic value. Living through the first decades of England's Industrial Revolution, an era which witnessed a major leveling of class status and income, and the birth of wide scale consumerism, Smith observed that people seemed to possess an innate desire to better themselves (Smith meant financially) which in turn was a driving force in the economy. Individual competition, according to Smith, automatically bettered the society as a whole, even if it led to a change in the form of government.15 Of course, modern interpretations of Smith's doctrine of laissez faire capitalism continue to influence policy Inakers in Europe and the United States today. In more Inodern times, however, economists have talked increas- lingly in terms of supply and demand, price systems, lltility, marginal productivity, business cycles and .,. . \ vx A..- $.‘: 49 would be defined as "one that permitted man's highest purposes to emerge by satisfying all his prepotent 23 In addition, Maslow believes his basic needs.” theory has important practical relevance for child development, psychology, sociology, cultural history and numerous specific clinical and academic research problems. Though Maslow's theory does not assign a lower moral level to his lower needs and a higher morality to his higher needs, it is interesting that Maslow's need hierarchy and Kohlberg's stages of values development contain some parallels from stage to stage. Without advancing at this time to a full-scale treatment of Kohlberg, it is possible to say that both men rate hedonistic and less-reciprocal needs or values lower than those which involve social interaction with and approval by another person or group of persons. Also for both men, approval by the society or larger system, called "self—esteem" in Maslow's case, is ranked higher than approval by the family or in-group (called "love-belonging“ in the Maslow scheme). Finally, both men see the highest values as very complex, involving a deep self-knowing on the part of the individual. Comments on Scheler and Maslow have been presented at some length since these philosophers developed hierarchical theories in some respects like _ ‘ .- ... ruuuvl “...-” u . 'I..." ma... I -‘ o.-. o 5‘}: . I “ 50 Kohlberg's. Since both Scheler and Maslow maintained that some values are higher than others, each implicitly suggested how one ought_to value. Maslow, however, - thought that environmental constraints beyond the I individual's control could get in the way of higher need satisfaction. In fact, in his various references to environmental determinants, Maslow revealed himself not too far distant from the most influential group of value theorists in the United States at the present time-~the behavioral scientists. For over fifty years, "behaviorism," as it is called, has had the type of multi-discipline influence in this country that existentialism enjoyed in most of Europe.24 In some respects, behaviorism is a uniquely narcissistic philosophy, which no doubt owes much of its success to the climate of Optimism and prosperity which has long been the trademark of American living. Nomothetic in the extreme, behaviorism relies on the idea that human develOpment in general is a function primarily of the environment. The fundamental principle of behaviorism is that the human mind is a reflection of the external world. Although behavioral scientists do not view themselves as cultural relativists, the fact that behaviorism has "taken off" in this country to an extent not matched in any other (with the exception, ...-‘- 'v . own“ Ov- -- 9" Q. ‘ .5 or .1 ‘V W“. 1‘ n.5, 5-. .’. “‘5 .....- ‘f'oa . ...... n In... . . u- U.- I 9-... . .. ._v—. _ .'.‘N¢u ‘7- (I) ..‘-. v \' -. ‘Ug .: . v. i - -.. “ ~~t ‘ ~ D a ‘- b‘.‘...‘~ “ . x u . . . vv'x. -,~ I ‘- “A N5 ‘5 51 interestingly enough, of the U.S.S.R.) suggests that behaviorism is, in fact, socially normative, and that it is the goal of behaviorists, whether acknowledged or not, to transpose the forms of the environment onto, the individual. It is significant, I believe, that the prime targets for behavioristic studies are children, deviant adolescents, the mentally retarded, those with learning disabilities, the incarcerated, and other groups of individuals considered potentially patho- logical by the society. (Recently, this includes college students.) For the prime directive of behavior- ism is to change people. The branches of behaviorism in this country are myriad, but all of them have as their goal to change individual values and behavior, or to better understand hgw_human values and behavior are changed. The fact that the general educated public is not threatened by this is due, I believe, to the culturally normative aspect of behaviorism mentioned above. Most behaviorists are system-oriented, interested in pushing people toward the norms of our society, not away from them.25 Behaviorists in psychology, for example, largely have as their goal the "adjustment" of indi- viduals to the society's view of right and prOper. A particularly fruitful branch of the movement, called the social learning theory branch, is a merger of ‘ turn-Ans - I o can inugu‘ Ou-VQIU.bgd a I-I-u.. ' 4 ...-ol.‘. . ‘ "'-:r~ .. fem—...... . ' n ‘ n In. '.. ' u it...“_'. . M an—. o . a} ’~.‘~ ' vu-‘b_~‘ .- n" V “ a. c.» l 5 ~ a C... V? . 52 psychoanalysis and behaviorism which relies on guilt (including remorse, pain and anxiety) to motivate "moral" behavior. Likewise in education, behaviorists are primarily interested in procedures that will motivate children to respond better to the behavior patterns expected by the school, especially the discipline con- sidered necessary for an atmosphere in which the teacher can best function. An impressively large body of literature by behaviorists has been concerned with understanding "attitudes" and attitude change. Although recently there has been some concern to distinguish between "attitudes" and "values," for my purposes, the "attitude" literature can be considered to be about values as well. The literature reveals the difficulty behaviorists have in measuring the existence of certain attitudes, and their even greater difficulty in proving that subjects under- went meaningful attitude change as a result of some experience generated by researchers. The "self report" method, which utilizes a written or oral examination, is commonly used but it is considered unreliable by many social scientists. In addition, studies using this measure are often open to the charge of triviality, especially when subjects are required to choose between or rank fairly distinct and unrelated items, without any opportunity to explain their reasoning process. The only indication of attitude change which is given high . . .7. the A.;-: >- h ‘ uvichi‘ v . I-u. ‘vk. “u- . "V. is. .1 . 0-- ' I \(‘ .... hut...‘ I u ‘s. .A., p F DU~..¥.'. ~ in:. .;. I“ ’0 " Nu‘ z'lvb .‘ -“. w “2. ..., eh.“ y“ . :.:'s ‘ ~u-..‘ ~ . - I‘o. ..- H ... kt. . Q ~‘ "‘ v. i: ‘ “an . ~v.‘ . ._ .-‘ A»- $.. ~b“ s:(;- .M‘I‘.‘ 53 credence by many behaviorists is prolonged change in the subject's behavior. The difficulty in proving attitude change apart from behavior has led "radical behaviorists" of the B.F. Skinner persuasion to assume that neither values nor 26 According to Skinner, behavior is attitudes exist. completely determined by environment and there is nothing that the individual can do about it, since the forces that will direct his life have been set in motion before he was ever born. Values, for Skinner, have no reality apart from behavior; they are merely a form of rational- ization we have developed to help us feel that we under- stand the environmental forces that direct our lives. Few behaviorists are as extreme as Skinner in their view of values, but most behaviorists believe that one of the best ways to change beliefs is to first force a change in behavior. To summarize briefly, previous theories have taken either a nomothetic or idiographic approach toward values, maintaining in mutually exclusive terms that value is an aspect of the environment, or a creation of the mind of the valuer, respectively. Some idiographic theories have claimed that although value does exist in the environment, it only comes into being when it is invested with interest/appetite/desire, etc., on the part of the valuer. By denying the existence of intrinsic 54 value, idiographic theories, however, have in general failed to suggest how one ought to value, or which values are in any sense better than others. As a result, meaningful analysis of values based on their theories is impossible. Nomothetic theorists such as Max Scheler and Abraham Maslow, who have attempted to set up hier— archies of personal values, have provoked much discussion and some additional research. Since their theories were not empirically derived in the first place, however, it is difficult to validate them without being subject to the charge of loading the dice. Therefore, values analysis based on such allegedly absolute scales is highly questionable. Finally, the claim has been made that values analysis conducted by behavioral scientists has been limited, of questionable validity and that it has been more concerned with behavior, and specifically behavior change, than with values for their own sakes. Since the assumption of many critics that the mass media reflect and influence values is essentially behavior- istic in nature, however, a review of behaviorist research on the impact of TV viewing seems a logical starting point for an analysis of values associated with television. A case study of the behavioristic method, as it has been applied to television, is therefore the subject of Chapter 3. '- -... ..v-.-' ... ‘ b y“. 55 Notes to Chapter 2 1See John S. Stewart, Toward a Theory for Values Development Education, Ph.D. dissertation (Michigan State University, 1974), 25-26. The terms "nomothetic" and "idiographic" are used by Stewart in his descriptions of the modern-day values philosophies which the Kohlberg theory synthesizes and partially incorporates. The present analysis extends these definitional terms to apply to value theories of the past. The Stewart terms were used primarily to avoid wordy repetitions of the concepts they are defined to represent. 2See Jeremy Bentham, The Principles of Morals and Legislation, ed. Laurence Lafleur (New York, 1948). A good biography of Bentham is Mary Peter Mack, Jeremy Bentham: An Odyssey of Ideas (New York, 1963). 3Nietzche's critique of Christianity and his proposed alternative in self—conscious strength of will are outlined most comprehensively in Friedrich W. Nietzsche, The Will to Power, translated and edited by Walter Kaufmann and R.J. Hollingdale (New York, 1968). The Will to Power was first published in 1901. 4A good critique of Brentano, Meinong, von Ehrenfels and Perry in terms of their contributions to value theory is found in R. Frondizi, What Is Value? An Introduction to Axiology, 2nd ed. (La Salle, Ill., 1971), 33-47. 5Bertrand Russell, Religion and Science (New York, 1935), 137-143. 6Frondizi, What Is Value?, 111. 7Rollo Handy, The Measurement of Values: fighavioral Science and Philosophical Approaches (St. Louis, 1970). Handy also recognizes the mutually exclusive characteristic of "subjective" versus "objective" approaches to values analysis; he recommends a "transactional" approach. 81bid., 52. t“ 5.. q ll.'~ I“. . nun-c b-" u.» « .. n...~'|- ...-......A' ‘ \ . ... p .. . " w . - ~55.‘.5 -.- ---p-.. ' K n w I..- b.u~.~‘ - o , ..-. 't-‘n— "' out»... .‘*¢ . . a 'ovau..\.n . . -:~~«- .. '- ““U‘ an . In..... ‘_~ .- vu...b' '. .C c. -:P-.- .- 56 9Cultural relativism is not too different from nomothetic philosophy, since cultural values are obligatory for the members of a society. Underlying the beliefs of many cultural relativists in this country one suspects, is the conviction that American society represents a peak in cultural evolution, so that violation of U.S. norms is absolutely "wrong." The native in Africa, however, can be forgiven if he or she chooses to convert to Western standards. 10Louis E. Raths, Sidney B. Simon, and Merril Harmin, Values and Teaching: Working with Values in the Classroom (Columbus, Ohio, 1966), 10. llIbid., 28-30. 12Ibid., 114—115. 13A good discussion of mercantilism is found in Robert Lekachman, A History of Economic Ideas. 14Ibid. Other good sources of general informae tion on economic theories of value include Joseph Dorfman, The Economic Mind in American Civilization, Vol. I-III (New York, 1946-1949) and Robert Heilbroner, The Worldly Philosophers (New York, 1953). 15Ibid. Smith's The Theory of Moral Sentiments (London, 1911), which was first published in 1759, is also interesting in the present context. A rewarding comparison could perhaps be made between Smith's theory of morality and Lawrence Kohlberg's theory of values development since Smith specifically dwells on appre- hensions regarding "approbation" and "duty" as central to moral perceptions. Such terms are very suggestive of the language Kohlberg uses in describing the character- istics of his Stages 3 and 4 of values development. 16The reasoning involved in Kant's categorical imperative is discussed in Lewis White Beck, A Commen- tary on Kant's Critique of Practical Reason (Chicago, 1960). Further formulations of the imperative, including the familiar maxim to "treat humanity in every case as an end, never as a means only," are dis- cussed in Harry Burrows Acton, Kant's Moral PhilOSOphy (London, 1970). .‘a N .... ..., I‘! ..‘~,‘ ....1 (.... u. u A. ...... .'.~ I' "' "U be.-. - 0w; “ I. ""- .u g ... m...“ h ‘ ... -__ \ In ‘ ...... __‘. '~ a».‘_“‘ .-’I"R~ .- uh...".l . .... ‘ fl _‘ ~‘..‘_ 5 ‘ . u. - :- ”~5 be... H. q. ‘_ - '\ ..v_. a. ..‘._. a...~ " ‘ A v... ‘.‘ v ‘ 9...... \n-H .‘ "bn . ~;'I:“ ‘ "‘ ' a l 57 17George E. Moore, Principia Ethica (Cambridge, 1956), especially 131 ff. and 197 ff. 18Quotes are from William James, "What Prag- matism Means" in John K. Roth, ed., The Moral Philosophy gf William James (New York, 1969), 276; and James, "Pragmatism and Religion," ibid., 339. These essays were originally given as lectures at the Lowell Insti- tute in Boston in 1906. Classifying James as a "nomo- thetic" philosopher will perhaps meet with some objection since many scholars have previously labeled him a "relativist," considering pragmatism and radical empiricism to be relativistic doctrines. It is true that James' theories, like John Dewey's, are on the border- line between nomothetic and idiographic philosophy, as herein defined. In fact, James' ideas bear many similarities to structuralist-developmentalist thought. It seems to me, however, that James is primarily con- cerned to transpose the locus of value away from intangible absolutes (like Plato's "essences" and Kant s "categorical imperative") to concrete absolutes, which he calls "pure experience" in the world. In the essay which kicked off the pragmatism movement in America, "Does Consciousness exist?" (reprinted in Essays in Radical Empiricism, ed. R.B. Perry, New York, 1922), James explains his theory that consciousness and objective reality are not two substances with qualitative differences but rather that they form two parts of a whole, best understood in terms of their relationships with each other. The permutations of the argument lead frankly to;the conclusion, however, that the whole of which consciousness and objective reality are parts is really just objective reality, in James' opinion. James is concerned with the practical conse- quences of actions precisely because he is most certain of external reality. He is less sure about how or if individual minds vary (restructure) this reality. 19Scheler violently rejected both Kantian formalism and the various forms of relativism which were popular in the late nineteenth and early twentieth century. His doctrine of the hierarchy of moral values appears in Max Scheler, The Nature of Sympathy, trans. Peter Heath (London, 1954). In a companion book, Scheler accuses all those who hold subjective theories of value of being motivated by what he calls "ressenti- ment." See Scheler, Ressentiment, trans. William W. Holdheim (New York, 1961). Both books were originally written as essays in the 1910's. 58 20From The Nature of Sympathy, quoted in Frondizi, 81. 21The information on Scheler's hierarchy is summarized from Frondizi, 81-101. 22See especially Abraham H. Maslow, "A Theory of Human Motivation," Psychological Review, Vol. 50, 370-396 for a basic discussion of Maslow's need hier— archy. 23Ibid., 393. 24In the introduction to the revised edition of his book Behaviorism (New York, 1930), John B. Watson, the movement's founder, summarized some of the history of behaviorism in America. At first, the movement was very unpopular with the established elite in psychology and with moralists and religious idealists in general. Watson tries to explain why "behaviorism has had to weather such a continuous storm." He writes: "Behavior- ism, as I tried to develop it in my lectures at Columbia in 1912 and in my earliest writings, was an attempt to do one thing--to apply to the experimental study of man the same kind of procedure and the same language of description that many research men had found useful for so many years in the study of animals lower than man. We believed then, as we do now, that man is an animal different from other animals only in the types of behavior he displays.... I think the forcing of this conviction caused most of the storm."(ix) Watson goes on to brag, however, that "without being overtly accepted," behaviorism's influence had nonetheless become profound during the first 18 years of its existence: "To be convinced of this, one needs only to compare the contents of our journals title by title for 15 years before the advent of behaviorism and during the past 15 to 18 years. One needs only to compare the books written before and after. Not only have the subjects studied become behavioristic but the words of the presentations have become behavioristic. Today no uniVersity can escape the teaching of behavior- isnn In some its methods and hypotheses are accepted, in others it is taught ostensibly for the purpose of criticism. The truth is that the younger generation of students demands at least some orientation in behavior- ismm"(xi) 'O r;. - 59 25The following statements from the opening chapter of best seller Beyond Freedom and Dignity (New York, 1971), by leading behaviorist B.F. Skinner, illustrates the normative aspect of the discipline. "In trying to solve the terrifying problems that face us in the world today, we naturally turn to the things we do best. We play from strength, and our strength is science and technology. To contain a population explosion we look for better methods of birth control. Threatened by a nuclear holocaust, we build bigger deterrent forces and anti-ballistic- missile systems. We try to stave off world famine with new foods and better ways of growing them. Improved sanitation and medicine will, we hope, control disease.... But things grow steadily worse and it is disheartening to find that technology itself is in- creasingly at fault. "What we need is a technology of behavior. We could solve our problems quickly enough if we could adjust the growth of the world's population as pre- cisely as we adjust the course of a spaceship.... " (1-3) 26Ibid., especially 98-99. I. CM. ‘1 \ P. .‘1 .- 1' e u... ‘ . "~.. .n: “s—. I U) tn “v- ‘d ‘4 ‘y CHAPTER 3 BEHAVIORISM AND MEDIA ANALYSIS: THE SURGEON GENERAL'S REPORT ON TELEVISION AS A CASE STUDY OF THE BEHAVIORISTIC APPROACH As stated at the end of Chapter 2, the claim that the mass media can affect the values of its audience is essentially behavioristic in origin. To date, research into the effects of television viewing has been limited to behavioristic studies, the great majority of which have been concerned with the impact of TV violence viewing on children and adolescents. This concern with media violence could have been antici- pated. Beyond its impact on the individual, violence is seen as ultimately harmful for the life of the society as a whole. As is amply demonstrated on "crime drama“ programs on television, violence is associated with theft, larceny, fraud, embezzlement, blackmail and a host of other crimes that are dangerous to the functioning of a capitalistic society. Since behaviorists claim they can change values and behavior, which they believe are regulated entirely by the environment, it seems logical to assume that considering the advisability of media regulation is one goal underlying such studies of TV violence. Furthermore, it is no doubt fear of media regulation which has been responsible for the cool and qualified 60 KN ...... - . ‘ "Iodg. .‘ ‘. n"- .‘F' n.‘,."‘ -. ‘ .. v-.l‘”‘ . \ .. ‘ ... v '5‘ . . en ..‘ ._ '- ...- . A. .~_ . ~ _ ..“.» ‘3 A ' 0. .“.‘ a a : ‘~ ‘.“‘ U h u " y “-'~“ .F ..V ‘ I W: A- ‘ 9" 61 reception that even the strongest behaviorist studies have received from personnel representing both broad- casting and nonbroadcasting media. The present chapter will review the claims of the most recent and most exhaustive such behaviorist study, completed in early 1972 by the United States Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior.1 The Surgeon General's study represents the culmination of a slowly growing concern over the impact of television violence, which began in the early years of the medium's popularity. Some of the earliest studies of television were content analyses which attempted to document the extent of violence on TV. An early study conducted in New York City in 1952 discovered that 3,000 acts of violence had been portrayed in a single week.2 In 1956, a subcommittee of Senator Kefauver's Crime Investigation Committee reported, amidst general committee findings of rising urban crime, that juvenile deliquency might well be related to viewing of violent programs on television. Although TV violence continued to increase during the late fifties and early sixties, national concern over the relationship between violence viewing and aggression remained essentially dormant until the youth riots of the middle and late sixties.3 Desperately searching for causes of widespread teenage violence, President Johnson's National Commission on 62 the Causes and Prevention of Violence concluded in 1969 that television was in part responsible, especially in precipitating violence by children from poor and broken homes.4 Finally, President Nixon, elected on a "law and order" ticket, accepted the suggestion of Senator Pastore that a full scale investigation be conducted by the Surgeon General's office to determine exactly what, if any, harmful effects TV has on children and adolescents. After a careful screening procedure, in which the TV networks themselves participated (this was greatly resented by the Committee members when it came to their attention) a group of well-known behavioral scientists, together with a sprinkling of media rep- resentatives, was appointed by the Surgeon General.5 In an attempt to move as quickly and efficiently as possible, the Committee subcontracted its studies to a number of researchers across the country, many of whom had projects in progress at the time the Committee was formed. In early 1972, the Committee was ready with its results--contained in a summary document written by Committee members and five lengthy supporting volumes 0f papers, recording the findings of each individual PrOject conducted. The Committee's first problem, it revealed, was in coming to a consensus as to what constitutes 63 "violence“ and what a "violent act." Even regarding certain specific acts, they felt that circumstances and the age, sex and social role of participants made a difference. Nonetheless, they used the following as a working definition: "the overt expression of physical force against others or self, or the compelling of action against one's will on pain of being hurt or killed."6 This definition of violence is in complete accord with the primary characteristic, namely orienta— tion to punishment, of Kohlberg's lowest stage (Stage 1) of values development. The Committee recognized, how- ever, the ease with which personalized violence against a particular victim can be confused with violence justified in order to maintain society as a system: Almost every society, including primitive societies, legitimizes for the sake of its own maintenance some aggression and violence against internal and external threats. Every society has inconsistent norms and mores. Every society talks a better, purer, more noble game than it plays. Aggression and violence are always the legitimized privi- leges of authority, whether it be within the setting of the family, within a tribe, or within a nation. The possibility of screening violent acts and programs based on the motives of particular characters was apparently considered by the committee, but was evi- dently dismissed as not worth the effort. One study 64 conducted under the auspices of the Committee, more- over, showed that only older adolescents had a high capacity for understanding the motives behind various violent acts. Young children displayed little or no ability to distinguish motives.8 The actual research contracted by the Committee can be divided into two broad groups: studies of chil- dren and studies of adolescents. Since the techniques and measures used for these age groupings differ somewhat, it is best to discuss them separately, begin— ning with the research on children. Many studies have shown that children are capable of imitating any act viewed on the screen, so the Committee did not contract for any additional imitation studies. As the Committee put it, "The fact that children can mimic film-mediated aggressive behavior is perhaps the best-documented finding in the research literature on the effects of the pictorial media."9 The group selected instead projects which had as their goal to investigate "media instig- ation of aggressive behavior." When the research was completed, the Committee reported the general finding that “The majority of studies, covering various age levels, share the conclusion that viewing violence increases the likelihood that some viewers will behave aggressively immediately or shortly thereafter."10 Two 6“" 65 specific studies will be briefly described below to illustrate the techniques that were used. One study monitored the nursery school behavior of a group of 3-1/2 to 5—1/2-year-old boys and girls. Some of the children were shown aggressive programs, some neutral programs, and some pro-social programs. The study found that the greatest change in behavior was toward pro-social behavior in children from low socio—economic backgrounds who had viewed the pro-social material. In another study, aggressive and neutral programs, respectively, were shown to two groups of young children. In a laboratory experiment that fol- lowed, the children who had viewed the aggressive epi- sode were more prone to "hurt" rather than "help" an alleged child victim. In these and other studies of young children, direct observation of behavior in either a laboratory or nursery school setting was the most com- mon technique used to monitor change. Although this technique can be criticized as posing an artificial situation for children, behaviorists using this method claim that the children are given time to acclimate to the environment before testing. As a result, they say, the laboratory conditions are very close to school and other social environments that children normally experience.ll 66 For adolescents, the technique of self-reporting was used to draw correlations between violence viewing and aggression. Sometimes, evaluations made by a sub- ject's family and peers were used instead of the self- reports. The various measures that were used included 1) total time spent viewing television, 2) preference for violent programs, and 3) amount of time spent viewing violent programs. Although little correlation was ob- served between the measure of total time spent viewing and aggressive behavior, almost all the correlations between violence viewing and aggressive behavior were positive, most of them ranging from nil to .21. Significantly, the highest correlation coeffi- cients, both at about .30, involved studies in which earlier viewing was correlated with later aggressive ratings. In one study, mothers were asked to report on the viewing habits of their third grade sons. Then, ten years later, peer ratings of aggression were taken on these same boys. A .31 correlation was found between earlier exposure to TV violence and the peer ratings of aggression at age 18. Another study asked high school Students "how frequently they had watched each of thirteen shows that were on television three or four Years ago." This measure was then correlated with Current aggression, also measured by self-report. The 67 measure of past violence viewing correlated to aggression at .33, slightly above the correlation between the measure of current violence viewing and the measure of aggression (.30).12 Some interpreters of the Surgeon General's study, especially those connected with the media, have been quick to point out that the correlation factor in all the studies was at about .30, in the .20's or below. This, they claim, is not very convincing evidence that violence on televison is a significant factor in causing violent behavior. Other interpreters of the study, including some of those whose reports compose the technical sections of the document, argue that the recurrent appearance of positive correlations is sig- nificant and should not be overlooked.13 In evaluating the findings, the Committee itself is cautious and circular, presenting the case for both interpretations. It does concede, however, that "These data are suppor; tive of the interpretation that viewing leads to aggres- sion, within the parameters of a relationship at the .30 level."14 What ig the significance of a correlation at the .30 level? As the Committee points out, correlation is not the same as causation, nor does correlation necessarily imply that a causal sequence exists. Accor- ding to the Committee, causal inference requires: 68 1. Association--the variables must be shown to covary; 2. Time order--change in the specified cause must occur prior to change in the specified effect; and 3. Reasonable explanation or functional rela:. tionship in a non-mathematical sense. The Committee explains that correlation coefficients satisfy the first requirement and that those that link changes from earlier to later measurements also meet the second. But the third requirement, that of “reasonable explanation or relationship" can never be definitive, they say.15 Because of their association and time order, however, certain assumptions regarding the causal rela- tionship of two measures are standardly made by scien- tists. These assumptions are not discussed by the Committee. Specifically, taking the two variables, violence viewing and aggressive behavior, it can logi- cally be assumed, based on the tests administered by the subcontractees to the Surgeon General's Committee, that thirty per cent of the phenomenon "aggressive behavior" can be attributed to the factor "violence viewing." In addition, of course, about seventy per cent of the phenomenon aggressive behavior must be explained by other factors. Also, it is no doubt possible that the factors are related in complex, yet undiscovered, ways. by In 69 If it were the goal of the Committee to con- struct a model that would explain gll_the causes of aggressive behavior in children and adolescents, violence viewing, in itself, would obviously be insuf- ficient. However, considered as only one factor, it must be considered a very significant one. It is a naive view of cause and effect to consider that a single cause must account for all or even a major portion of the effect described in order to be significant.16 It was clearly the Committee's assigned task to describe television's contribution toward aggressive behavior, not to supply a check-list of all the causes and rank them according to their impact. In such a study, television might be comparatively less important. In the present study, however, it is television that has been indicted, and the indictment is strong whether or not media officials and the general public wish to admit it. In an attempt to qualify the results of the studies, the Committee explored at some length the possibility that a "common origin.variable" might be responsible for both violence viewing and aggressive behavior. Three potential candidates for such a vari— able emerged, in the Committee's Opinion. One was ”restrictive" (as opposed to physical or verbal) paren- tal punishment. In one study restrictive punishment 70 correlated to aggressive behavior at the .41 level, whereas violence viewing correlated with aggressive behavior at only the .17 level.17 According to the COmmittee, parental emphasis on nonaggression emerged as an even stronger candidate for a third variable, since children whose parents were definitely anti-violence were rated significantly lower in both biolence viewing and aggression. In a relation study, the failure of families to demonstrate disapproval was found to be more strongly related to aggressive attitudes than was exposure to television violence.18 The third candidate for a common origin variable discussed by the Committee was "family communi- cation patterns," but much additional research is needed in this area, according to the Committee.19 One of the questions which apparently plagued the Committee had to do with the mechanism whereby vio- lence viewing allegedly leads to aggression. This question was investigated by one of the research teams. It found correlations at about the .30 level between both violence viewing and aggression and three factors: 1) Identification with violent characters, 2) learning about aggression, and 3) linkage of television to real life. The second factor, however, "learning of aggres- sion" was further broken down into three categories: 71 1. Acquisition of knowledge about techniques, e.g., how to hit someone. 2. Acquisition of knowledge of pertinent facts of life, such as that hitting someone is in fact one way of gaining ends. 3. Acquisition of values, e.g., that hitting someone is a preferred way of gaining ends. The assumption was made by the researchers that aggres- sive behavior could not be stimulated by violence view- ing unless values favorable to violence were either learned in the process or had already been acquired. The correlations linking "approval of aggression" to violence viewing and to aggressive behavior, however, were the weakest found in the study. What this lower correlation (at about the .21 level) means is unclear, and the Committee suggests that further research into questions of value is called for.20 The Surgeon General's Committee took it for granted, however, that the mass media is one of many institutions that transmits values: We have noted and deplored the paucity of research about the manner in which values with respect to many areas of behavior, including violence, are transmitted, and about the role played by television and21 other mass media in this communication. The Committee also declared that "it is taken for granted that television programming is on the whole consonant with modal interests and values." This 72 assumption was made because television is a successful medium dependent upon a voluntary audience, but also because studies undertaken for the Committee clearly indicated that network personnel, including producers and writers of TV programs, consciously attempt to give the people what they want.22 This is the case because of the networks' desire to make money and to do it with as little risk as possible. Paul Monash, interviewed as part of a sympos- ium of TV writers, stated that almost all TV shows are derivative of others. Even coming up with a suggestion for a different program scheme doesn't guarantee that the network will buy it, according to Monash. Networks 23 In the prefer to remain with the tried and true. specific context of violence on television, one finding of the Surgeon General's staff was that there is a .49 correlation between the average Nielsen rating of programs classified as violent in any given year and the number of such programs broadcast in the following year. Another study found a .53 correlation between percent of programs classified as violent in any given season (prior to 1972) and the mean Nielsen ratings for 24 In other words, all evening programs in that season. the public tends to reward violent programming by increased viewing--a difficult incentive for a profit- oriented industry to resist. 73 The Committee leveled a more direct indictment at the broadcasting industry itself, however. It noted that given the voluminous amount of potential material, decisions must be continually made about what will be presented and: The values reflected in these decisions are no less relevant because they are generally unarticulated. The decisions made take on importance because all these varieties of television fare can structure the audience member's relationship to reality. To varying extents and in various ways, they can engage conscience, modify or mobilizgsopinion, and challenge or confirm beliefs. The Committee here very clearly holds out television's potential for encouraging values deve10pment. That TV does not do this is largely a matter of inertia and the profit motive, the Committee believes, but they isolate other factors as well. "In general, the powerful, influential, and elite have opportunity to initiate and control the content and uses of television in ways that the powerless, the poor, and the non-elite do not," they state.26 Recognizing that television has grave potential for perpetuating stereotypes about groups such as females, blacks, the poor, the elderly and so on, one of the goals of the present study is to determine what differences in values are attributed to men and women and to various age groups according to their represen- tation by television characters. npao. ~..I I RV 5‘ n] I u ‘n .A- a “A J vs ~'~. Q‘.‘ ~§ {7 ,2. all C ’I (1' g ,) ,’ 1 Ll ) 74 In summary, the Surgeon General's report is provocative and deserving of much greater public expo- sure than it has received. With regard to the limited task assigned the Committee, the findings are quite specific. To repeat, "These data are supportive of the interpretation that viewing leads to aggression, within the parameters of a relationship at the .30 level." Clearly, a .30 correlation should not be dismissed lightly, and in my opinion, the reason that it has been is largely due to fear of media regulation on the part of media officials and others.27 In terms of a more general analysis of values portrayed by televison, however, the type of procedure employed by the Surgeon General's Committee and its staff clearly has its limitations. To begin with, the amount of money, research and writing time that went into the analysis of a single aspect of television con- tent, namely televised violence, is overwhelming. Con- sider magnifying such a study by the number of poten— tially isolable values and behavioral traits that tele- vision could portray, assuming the level of thoroughness employed by the Surgeon General's Committee. The result- ing project would take a team Of researchers the size of the Committee's staff decades to complete. Perhaps such ongoing analysis of our most popular medium would be 75 beneficial, but to suggest it at the present time is clearly not feasible. In addition, the technique of self-report measures used by the Surgeon General's staff in the studies of adolescents has been seriously questioned by critics.28' This is so despite the fact that the staff was measuring such relatively concrete behavior as "violence viewing" and "aggression." The difficulties of constructing a credible self-report measure would increase dramatically if abstract values such as instrumental reward, peer approval, loyalty to the society, etc., or their associated behaviors, were to be considered. In terms of the present study, though, there are more profound, if more obvious, limitations to the behavioristic approach. Specifically, the method empha- sizes behavior, rather than underlying values, and socially malignant behavior at that. That is, behavior- istic measures are designed to pick up behavior that stands out as different, somehow deviant from the group or norm. Normative behavior and normative values are not questioned by behaviorists. As such, the whole issue of growth or personal development beyond the norm is not considered. Lawrence Kohlberg's studies indicate that input from the environment, which includes the mass media, can 76 (not mggt) influence behavior and personal growth, especially when it provides opportunities for role- taking, or taking on the perspectives of other people. Television, of course, provides constant opportunities for role taking on the part of viewers. Thus, in addition to its potential for negative influence, tele- vision has the capability to portray strong positive value models. Although the Surgeon General's Committee recognized television's potential for encouraging values development, it thought the profit motive prevented conscious utilization of this potential. I therefore consider it important to determine, according to Kohlberg's scale, the actual level of values development portrayed by the most popular television characters of recent years and to see how these levels compared with Kohlberg's estimates for the viewing population. An explication of Kohlberg's theory will be the subject of Chapter 4. 77 Notes to Chapter 3 1The findings of the study are summarized and critiqued in: The Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, Television and Growinngp: The Impact of Televised Violence (Washington, D.C., 1972). Five volumes of technICal documents were reviewed by the Committee in preparing the summary document. The five volumes are published separately as: U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare,Te1evision and Social Behavior: A Technical Repgrt to the Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Cdmmittegon Television and Social Behavior (Washington, D.E., 1972). It should be emphasized that the Surgeon General's study represents the most comprehenSiwestudy of televised violence using behavioral science methods. There are numerous books, pamphlets and dissertations on the subject as well. In part, the findings of a majority of these earlier studies that television violence can affect behavior prompted the massive government project coordinated by the U.S. Surgeon General. 2Dallas W. Smyth, New York Television: January 4-10, 1952 (New York TV Monitoring Study No. 4, Urbana, Illinois: N.A.E.B., 1952). 3The Kefauver findings are referenced and the increase in televised violence during the late 1950's and early 1960's is documented in U.S. Senate Committee on the Judiciary, Hearings Before the Subcommittee to Investigate Juvenile Delinquency (Washington, D.C.; 1962, 1964), Parts 10 and 16. 4David M. Rein, "The Impact of Television Violence," Journal of Popular Culture, 7 Part 4 (Spring 1974), 934. 5The Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee, Television and Growing Up, 21-24. 61bid., 5. 71bid., so, 190. 81bid., 96, 114-116. 78 9Ibid., 103. 101bid., 106. 11See Aletha H. Stein and Lynette K. Friedrich with Fred Vondracek, "Television Content and Young Children's Behavior," U.S. Dept. of NEW, Television and Social Behavior, Vol. II, 202-317; and Robert M. fiebert and Robert A. Baron, "Short-term Effects of Televised Aggression on Children's Aggressive Behavior," Ibid., 181-201. A laboratory playroom is described in Television and Growing Up, n. 63. 12See Monroe M. Lefkowitz, Leonard D. Eron, LeOpold 0. Walter and L. Rowell Huesmann, "Television Violence and Child Aggression: A Followup Study," Television and Social Behavior, Vol. III, 35-135; and Jac, M. McLeod, Charles K. Atkin and Steven H. Chaffee, "Adolescents, Parents, and Television Use: Self-Report and Other-Report Measures from the Wisconsin Sample," Ibid., 257, 299. 13Rein, 943. 14Television and Growing Up, 161. 15Ibid., 143-144. 16Often the mere absence of negative correla- tion as was the case with all the reported studies, is considered "significant" relationship between two variables. 17Television and Growing Up, 169-170. The study referencedgis Jack M. McLeod et a1., cited above. 18Ibid., 170-171. Four studies cited include two by McLeod et al.; Steven H. Chaffee and Jack M. McLeod, “Adolescent Television Use in the Family Context"; and Joseph R. Dominick and Bradley S. Green- berg, "Attitudes Toward Violence: The Interaction of Television Exposure, Family Attitudes, and Social Class," all in Television and Social Behavior, Volume III. 79 19Television and Growing Up, 171-172. 2°Ibid., 159. 21Ibid., 189. 22The studies referenced include George Gerbner, "Violence in Television Drama: Trends and Symbolic Functions"; Muriel G. Cantor, "The Role of the Producer in Choosing Children's Television Content?; and Thomas F. Baldwin and Colby Lewis, "Violence in Tele- vision: The Industry Looks at Itself," all in Television and Social Behavior, Volume. I. 23A. William Bluem and Roger Manvell, eds., Television: The Creative Experience; A Surygy of Anglo-American Progress (New York, 1967), 67. 24See David G. Clark and William B. Blankenburg, "Trends in Violent Content in Selected Mass Media," Television and Social Behavior, Vol. 1, 188-243. 25Television and Growinggp, 42—43. 26Ibid., 44-45. See also p. 79. The Committee writes, "Although many among network personnel express interest in reducing violence in thier programs, they feel constrained by the economic realities of broadcasting." 27It is possible, in addition, that "violent" characters on television, particularly the law enforcers, exhibit the values of network executives in another form. Network officials may privately laud such aggressiveness, when associated with professional expertise, tenacity in problem solving, "getting the jump" on the other guy and so forth. As will be explained in Chapter 7, the most violent regular characters on television are also the most "professional“ and the most conventionally moral, using the Kohlberg scale. CHAPTER 4 LAWRENCE KOHLBERG'S THEORY OF VALUES DEVELOPMENT Throughout each of the preceding chapters, I have stressed the need for a philosophically sound but scien- tifically derived theory of value which can be prac- tically applied to the study of television characters. Chapter 2 discussed a number of philosophers who attempted to define and analyze values based on theories conceived apart from scientific investigation into the nature of human preferences and needs. I decided, however, that such hypothetical structures are shaky tools for analysis of the values of real individuals or for those of people as they are portrayed by the media. Since the claim that the mass media can reflect and influence values is behavioristic in origin, Chapter 3 then examined the behaviorist approach to values measurement as it has been applied to the study of television. The United States Surgeon General's analysis of the relationship between violence viewing and aggressive behavior was used as a case study of this approach. Although the Surgeon General's report contained provocative findings, I concluded that only the effect of media-reflected values associated with concretely observable behavior, such as aggression, can be efficiently measured by the .behaviorist method. This is so because of the behaviorist 80 81 need to prove that actual changes in behavior have occurred in subjects as a result of some previous atti- tude or value change. Having eliminated other theories of value and approaches to values analysis as impractical and inade- quate for the present study, I now wish to turn to an explication of Lawrence Kohlberg's Theory of Values Development. The present chapter will explain the Kohlberg theory in detail, thus providing both the rationale for its selection and the background informa- tion prerequisite to an understanding of its application to the analysis of the values of television characters. The Theory of Moral Judgment (Values Development) advanced by Lawrence Kohlberg is the only current theory of values development that is based on longitudinal, cross-cultural studies of nondeviant adolescents and adults. Kohlberg theorizes that there are certain principles (different from rules or laws) that are cental to "right” human values and behavior. His studies showed that subjects in different societies move sequentially through each of three levels, including six hierarchical stages of moral deve10pment. The levels and stages of values development according to Kohlberg's theory are summarized in Table 1.1 Kohlberg's research indicates that individuals pass through stages sequentially, without the possibility of skipping any stage except "Stage" 4%. In addition, TABLE 1 From L. Kohlberg's Theory of Moral Judgment: Classification of Moral Judgment into Levels and Stages of Development Levels Basis of Moral Stages of Development Judgment I Goodness and Stage 1. The_punishment and badness are obedience orientatiOn. The seen in terms physical consequences of of their action determine its goodness physical con- or badness. Avoidance of sequences as punishment and unquestioning determined by deference to power are values those concrete in their own right, not in persons who terms of respect for an enunciate the underlying moral order E22- rules and ported by punishment and labels of authority (the latter is good and bad Stage 4). Stage 2. The instrumental relagivist orientation or the "business ethic." Right action consists of that behavior which satisfies one's own needs and ocassion- ally the needs of others. Fairness, reciprocity, and equal sharing are valued by the Stage 2 individual, but they are always interpreted in a physical, pragmatic way. Reciprocity is a matter of "you scratch my back and I'll scratch yours," not loyalty, gratitude or justice. II Maintaining Stage 3. The interpersonal the expecta- tions, rules and standards of the family group or nation are seen as valuable in their own concordance, or “good girl7good bgy" orientation. Good behavior is what pleases or helps others and is therefore approved by them. There is much conformity to stereo- typical images of what is majority or "natural" behavior. Table 1 (continued) Levels Basis of Moral Judgment 83 Stages of Development II cont. right. There is a concern not only with conforming to the individual social order but also in maintaining, justifying and supporting this order. Imitation and identification are common. Society as a whole is viewed somewhat vaguely, and the perspective is dyadic, oriented toward the small group or toward a small community of peers. Stage 4. The "law and order" orientation. The individual is oriented toward authority, fixed rules, and the mainten- ance of the social order. Right behavior consists of doing one's duty, showing respect for authority, and maintaining a stable social system and one's own character. The individual at Stage 4 sees things from the perspective of the public or the gener- alized member of society who belongs to several groups or mutual dyads, and who has developed a perspective toward that which is common to all groups, i.e., to society as a system. Stage 4}. The stage of cynical ethical relativism. This is not a true stage, since not everybody goes through it on the way to principled thinking. At Stage 4%, the person has developed the Stage 4 capacity to take society's point of view, understands the nature of society, but now rejects the claim of Stage 4 to prior- ity and validity. Seeing society's point of view as not necessarily valid, the Stage 4% individual goes on to question the validitv of all moral views and concludes that everything is relative. Table 1 (continued) 84 Levels Basis of Moral Stages of Development Judgment III Major thrust Stage 5. The social contract is toward orientation. Laws that are autonomous moral principles that have validity apart from the authority of the group or concrete individuals. The principled thinker is able to evaluate the moral validity of concrete social rules and norms, through tests of more general justice principles. not constitutional, that vio- late human rights or that are not in the general interest are judged to be invalid. Since there are two distinct substages of Stage 5, their descriptions follow. Stage 5-A. Thegprocess orientation. This stage corre- sponds closely to Bentham's notion of the "greatest good for the greatest number." There is a clear awareness that personal values and opinions are relative and a corresponding emphasis upon procedural rules for reaching consensus. Stage 5-B. Human liberty orientation. Self-development and perfection of individuals as human beings are the highest priorities. The rights of individuals take precedence over the societal perspective. Stage 6. The universal ethical principle orientation. Orien- tation is towards ethical principles appealing to logical consistency, compre- hensiveness and universality. The principles are abstract, and at Stage 6, the individual evaluates, in terms of ideal reciprocity, the rights, welfare and dignity of all individuals. That course of action which benefits the least advantaged person(s) is seen as most just 85 his data shows that subjects tend to act out the highest stage of moral development they can understand, although they have the capacity to be taught the principles of the next highest stage. Since there are relatively few models of principled thinking in the United States or in any other culture (Kohlberg believes that less than fifteen percent of the population of the United States reaches Stage 5, with perhaps only five percent reaching Stage 6), Kohlberg thinks it likely that most individuals reach Stage 6 only through the intense stimulation provided by personal crisis and sustained responsibility for the welfare of other people.2 Kohlberg's theory explicitly rejects as incom- plete and partially incorrect both the nomothetic and idiographic approaches discussed in Chapter 2. It will be recalled that the nomothetic approach conjectures that values are located in the external world. On the other hand, the idiographic approach claims that values are the internal and subjective creation of the individual valuer. In preference to the mutually exclusive . nomothetic and idiographic theories of value, Kohlberg, his mentor Jean Piaget and other members of their school, have adopted a philosophy and methodology which they describe as structural-developmental. The struc- tural-developmental approach to values involves views of the human organism which stand in contrast to both 86 behaviorism and relativism, modern-day nomothetic and idiographic disciplines, respectively, while at the same time mediating between them.3 As a result of cross-cultural research, the structural-developmentalists have discovered that there is an underlying universality to the way humans organize thought, use logic, define situations and take social perspectives. They recognize that the superficial aspects of human behavior are relativistic, being to a great extent culturally and situationally determined. The great discovery is that underneath overt behavior there is an underlying structure which is responsible for the fact that individuals in all cultures think, act out and evaluate given situations using the same type of reasoning_process when they are at corresponding stages of mental and moral development. Of prime importance to the structural-develop- mental approach is the view of humanity as "organismic."4 The organismic view supposes that one of the basic axioms of existence is that motivation is intrinsic to living organisms. In other words, the organism is not a machine, which requires some external force to set it in motion; the source of and force motivating its activity is internal to it. Some further characteristics of the organismic approach include: 87 - the study of man as a functional whole - the study of man in the healthy state rather than basing views on pathological states (a propensity of behaviorism) - a stress on man as intrinsically active and motivated, which includes the premise that man transforms any external reality in the process of perceiving and assimilating it. 5 On its surface, of course, this view stands in sharp contrast to behaviorism. As John Stewart, a leading interpreter of Kohlberg puts it: It is the view than an organism plus life is the foundation for understanding human behavior that leads to the incomplete, inadequate, and partially erroneous con— ceptualization of man, motivation, and social- ization in behaviorism. It is, for example, from this idea that the notion of external motivation and the need for the transmission of the structure of the external world to the child originates. The holistic (organismic) view leads to the idea that the organism participates in the growing and learning process. It is the difference between values acggisition versus values development. 6 In other words the organism both acts upon Egg is acted upon by its environment. This proposition provides the sought-for linkage and amalgamation of the two seemingly mutually exclusive idiographic and nomothetic theories of value. As a foundation for his research into values development, Kohlberg began with the theory of human 88 intelligence first enunciated by the famous Swiss psychologist and structuralist, Jean Piaget.7 Piaget theorizes that human intelligence consists of three prts: function, structure and content. Each of these aspects of intelligence interrelates with each other and with the environment. Function is the biologically invariant, genetically-determined part of intelli- gence; content is the most external and observable aspect of intelligence (including thought, language and behavior); and structure lies between. Although it is the structural aspect of human intelligence which most directly concerns Kohlberg, further explanation of func- tion and content is a necessary prerequisite in order to fully illuminate the notion of structure. According to Piaget, there are two broad functions which are genetically-determined parts of intelligence-- organization and adaptation. As the word would imply, organization is the underlying systematic pattern of relationships that makes it possible for the mind to inter- pret and process the information it receives. Adaptation is subdivided into two parts--assimilation and accommo- dation. Since "adaptation," "assimilation," and "accomo- dation" are common words in the English vocabulary, often used synonymously, it is necessary to pay careful attention to Piaget's technical definitions of these terms. By assimilation,Piaget means the process whereby 89 the mind receives data from the world in terms of already existing knowledge and familiar patterns. It is the aspect of adgptation by which the world is transformed to fit the organism. Broadly speaking, it involves regis- tering of new data into previously existing categories of the mind which make sense of the new data. Accomodation, on the other hand, is the process responsible for creating new cognitive categories into which raw data can be assimilated. In other words, it is the process by which the environment operates on the organism and forces the mind to change in order to accommodate to the world. The process whereby the mind keeps input balanced between assimilation and accommodation is called equilibrium. The example commonly used to illustrate the dif- ference between assimilation and accommodation has to do with the manner in which children learn to name and classify animals. After a child has learned the word "dog," she will try it out on all the house and lawn pets she sees and, by gaining the adult nod of approval, will learn to identify several types of animals as "dogs." In performing this task, she is using the process of assimilation, since she is learning a variety of types of the same class of animals, all of which fit into a previously learned category. The first time the child sees a cat, however, she will call i; a "dog" as well. When her parent corrects her, however, she will 90 create an entirely new category of animals--those called "cat." As opposed to the genetically-determined func- tions, organization and adaptation, content is the super- ficial manifestation of structure in the world. Content consists of thought, language and behavior, all of which are environmentally determined in the sense of being limited by the options provided by any given culture. The difference between content and its underlying struc- Eggg can best be illustrated through an example. Con- sider stealing. Now,even though stealing is thought to be wrong in almost every culture,nonetheless,some people continue to steal. They do sq,howeven.for a variety of reasons. By referring to Kohlbergs six stages of moral development,outlined in Table 1, it is possible to see how the logic,or structure,of any of the six stages could be employed in considering stealing the "right" thing to do. The individual who steals because he is fearful of being punished if he does not is operating at Stage 1, the lowest stage of moral development. The individual who steals because he "needs" a given item or in order to "even the score," as when employees leaving a company take extra office supplies to make up for "long hours of overwork," is operating at Stage 2. The individual who steals because it is helpful to and reinforced by his peer group is operating at Stage 3. It is unlikely that 91 a Stage 4 individual would steal since it is against the law, but if he did, it would be because some official of the society ordered him to do so in the best interests of the society. (This was the alleged orientation of most of the defendants in the Watergate political scandal of 1973-74.) A Stage 5 or 6 individual would steal in order to preserve or gain constitutional or human rights of individuals or in order to fulfill a human responsi- bility for an individual not able to act on his own behalf. For example, a Stage 5 individual might steal a high-priced drug from an uncompromising pharmacist in order to save the life of an impoverished loved one--or of a stranger for that matter. Thus it can be seen that whereas the content of each of these acts, namely stealing, was the same, the structure, or rational thought process behind the action varied. In their emphasis on structure or the underlying lggig_behind language and behavior, structural-develop- mentalists are again distinguished from behaviorists, who focus only on the content, or most superficial if most easily measured aspects of human action and intel- ligence. This leads directly to a discussion of structure. Piaget's general theories of structure grew out of his work with the structure of human intelligence. He startled the scientific world in the 1930's with his 92 discovery that children are not mini-grownups, slowly learning all the adult wisdom of the world, but that rather they are structural organisms in the process of transformation, who think qualitatively differently from adults.8 After extensive research with infants, children and adolescents, Piaget set forth his discovery of "stage development," the process whereby the structure of the human mind undergoes transformation so as to attain successively higher (more complex) levels of intellectual capability. A summary of Piaget's stages of cognitive development together with a summary of the characteristics of each stage appears in Table 2.9 It is a significant property of human intel- ligence that, as the individual passes through the life span, the functions, organization and adaptation.remain the same but the structures change. The functions, in constant interaction with the environment, gather and process the data which makes intellectual/moral develop- ment a possibility. When development occurs, it does so according to the genetically-determined transformation laws of invariance, sequence and hierarchy. In them-‘ selves, the laws are not responsible for the timing of structural change (development), though they determine the order of the change. The structures, on the other hand, are the logical reasoning processes contained within (and defining) each stage of intellectual or moral Piaget Stages 93 TABLE 2 of Cognitive Development Stage Name General Definition of Stage I. II. III. Sensorimotor Preoperational Concrete Operations Lasts from birth to approximately 18 months to 2-1/2 years. The child is born with reflexes which he coordinates into motor habits and perceptions. He then learns to repeat and vary acts. He discovers new means of behavior through experimentation. At the end of this stage, symbolic representation, or language, begins. Lasts from 2 or 3 years of age to 7 or 8 years. .The child's reasoning is transductive, that is, he reasons from particular to particular, not from general to particular (deduc- tion), or particular to general (induction). The child's thinking is also syncratic in that the sequence of events is interpreted as having causal significance. If A happened just before B, then A caused B. The child's thinking is further characterized by realism, or the belief that his point of view is also everyone else's point of view; artificialism, or the belief that all things and events are caused by people; and animism, or the belief that inanimate objects 'are‘alive. Lasts from 7 or 8 years of age to 11 or 12 years. Child can perform concrete operations, involving logical reasoning. These include classification, seriation, number- ing, combining, separating, repeating, dividing and substitut- ing--but these can be applied only to objects considered real, not to hypothetical objects. The child is no longer dominated by egocentrism 94 Table 2 (continued) Stage Name General Definition of Stage III. Concrete and can take the View of others. Operations The child becomes truly social and (continued) can cooperate in a truly reciprocal 33y. A development of primary importance during this period is conservation, or the ability to hold constant certain features, dimensions, qualities and charac- teristics of an object or situation when another aspect changes. IV. Formal Generally begins about 11 or 12 Operations years of age and may be well developed by 15 or 16, though some people never reach the formal operational stage at all. The person can perform operations upon operations, considering the form ' of an argument rather than only its content (the abstract rather than the concrete). Hypothetico- deductive reasoning, or reasoning about possibilities with conclusions drawn about the probable outcome, is possible. The person under- stands causation and can deal with PrOportion, analogy and inference. This period of development takes place relatively slowly and probably passes through at least three substages. ‘- .0 ‘I .1 UI III 95 development. An individual is "in" only one stage at a given time, however, though all the capabilities of the previous stages are retained. Thus, structure changes throughout life, although the functions (organization and adaptation) and the laws (invariance, sequence, hierarchy) which generate structure remain unchanged. Kohlberg can truly consider his moral development stages as separate structures, not only as a result of his deliberate coordination with the Piaget stages, but also because, as Stewart puts it, "they define and represent deep, underlying structures of thought pat- terns, logic, and perspective that are used by people in the resolution of moral conflicts and dilemmas ... they are not content, they are not attitudinal, they are not choices. They reflect and manifest the core of one's moral nature."10 Kohlberg's laboratory method for determining the moral development level of subjects involves the posing of certain dilemmas, designed to arrive at their under- lying reasoning for considering a certain course of action as "right" or "better" than another. Subjects are questioned extensively to prove their rationale. The following is one of the dilemmas used by the Kohlberg staff. Joe is a 14-year-old boy who wanted to go to camp very much. His father promised him he could go if he saved up the money for it 96 himself. So Joe worked hard at his paper route and saved up the $40 it cost to go to camp and a little more besides. But just before camp was going to start, his father changed his mind. Some of his friends decided to go on a special fishing trip, and Joe's father was short of the money it would cost. So he told Joe to give him the money he had saved from the paper route. Joe didn't want to give up going to camp, so he thought of refusing to give his father the money.1l After being told the basic dliemma, each subject is asked a series of questions designed to test his level of values development with regard to certain issues. In the case of the above dilemma, the issues tested for include a) the basis of the father—son relationship and b) the basis for contract formation or "promising." The questions asked include the following:12 1. Should Joe refuse to give his father the money? Why? 2. Is there any way in which the father has a right to tell the son to give him the money? Why? 3. What is the most important thing a good father should recognize in his relation to his son? Why that? 4. What is the most important thing a good son should recognize in his relation to his father? Why that? 5. Why should a promise be kept? 6. What makes a person feel bad if a promise is broken? 7. Why is it important to keep a promise to someone you don't know well or are not close to? 97 In evaluating the answers of the subject, the following activity is looked for: - Demonstration of the making of a moral choice by employing a particular kind of structural logic genuinely representative of the charac- teristics of a particular stage. - Demonstration of the rejection of the logic of the earlier stages. - Focus on salient concerns for a particular stage. - Understanding and implementation of the justice concepts consistent with a particular stage. 13 Major concerns and characteristics associated with each stage of values development together with the specific structural logic associated with 1) justice, 2) concept of rights and 3) value of life appear in Table 3. Also included are the general age ranges associated with each stage.14 Although Kohlberg stresses that only one accurate observation is necessary to place a subject at a par- ticular stage, several are usually made to account for the fallibility of the examiner. In scoring, a subject is considered to be at the highest stage of development that he is capable of understanding and.1discussing. In practice, each person is more likely to have a modal stage, that is, one at which at least 50 percent of 15 judgments are being made. The remaining judgments 98 TABLE 3 Major Concerns and Characteristics of Kohlberg's Stages of Values Development Stage 1 -- The Punishment and Obedience Orientation Concern: Characteristics: View of Justice: Concepts of Rights: Value of Life: General Age Distribution: Consequences of actions. Avoids bad acts to avoid punishment. "An eye for an eye." Retaliation automatic, regardless of other's intentions or motives. "Being right" same as "having a right." (Because authority figure says so.) Moral value of life is the same as physical or social status value. No earlier than 5 or 6. Prominent throughout the grade school years; dominant in grades one through three. Stage 2 -- The Instrumental Relativist Orientation Concerns: Characteristics: View of Justice: Concept of Rights: The person's own needs and desires. Property rights of the individual. Self—centered need satisfaction. Naive (as opposed to philosophical) pleasure seeking. May sacrifice own ego needs for use and control of others. "Coming out even." There is some concern for the other, but mostly of the sort "You scratch my back and I'll scratch yours." Person can do what he wants to with himself and his property, even if this conflicts with the rights of others. 99 Table 3 (continued) Stage 2 -- The Instrumental Realtivist Orientation (cont.) Value of Life: Value of life is equal to its worth to its possessor. General Age 7 or 8 earliest; 9 or 10 likelier. Distribution: Dominant in later grade school years, and many people become fixated instrumental egoists, remaining at this stage all their lives. Stage 3 -- The Interpersonal Concordance Orientation Concerns: Harmonious in-group relationships. Approval by family or in-group. Characteristics: Person is "nice" to gain approval from in-group. The intentions of self and others are considered very important; they are overworked. Person is "good" to maintain mutual expectations. Uses adult moral cliches and stereotypes. Imitates and identifies with others. Person is "nice" to get along with others and to maintain the status quo of the small group. View of Justice: Fairness means making everyone directly involved happy. There is an attempt to "balance things out" in terms of love, affection, gratitude and immediate social relationships. Concept of Person can do what he wants, but he Rights: has no right to do "evil" as defined by the group. . n‘.‘ \.. (I. l 100 Table 3 (continued) Stage 3 -- The Interpersonal Concordance Orientation (cont.) Value of Life: Value of life equals the value of empathy or affection given to it by family members and others. General Age 10-11 earliest; 11-12 likelier. Distribution: Becomes predominant about 12 or 13 and is very important throughout junior high and high school years. No upper limit. Stage 3-A Prime -- The Authoritarian Orientation Concerns and Same as Stage 3 except that there is Characteristics: great concern about following the letter of the law to maintain order and respect for legitimate authority. Stage 3-A Prime differs from Stage 4 in that there is no clear concept of society as a system at 3-A Prime. Chains of command are viewed somewhat as small groups, and the feeling is that if any link fails or if any law is broken, all law and order may break down. The person is more concerned with chaos than with the disintegration of the system, per se. Stage 4 -- The Law and Order (or Conscientious) Orientation Concerns: A smoothly running society. Doing one's duty (and whether or not the other guy does his duty). Characteristics: Seeks to maintain social order for its own sake. Does his "duty," defined in terms of responsibilities doled out by the social order. Expects respect and other rewards of the society in return for doing duty. Opposes giving society's rewards to those who don't do their duty to the system. 101 Table 3 (continued) Stage 4 -- The Law and Order (or Conscientious) Orientation (cont.) Believes it necessary to "follow the rules" (norms of the society) in interpersonal relationships to maintain firmly established mutual expectations. View of Justice: In resolving competing claims, believes should do what's best for the society. Retributive justice is applied to "balance the scales." Equality equals uniform and regular administration of the laws. Any offender must pay his "debt to society." The individual's situation should be considered in the context of the larger system ("What if everyone...?"). Concept of Rights equal legitimate claims earned Rights: by contribution to the society. Value of Life: Believes "Thou shalt not kill" but this is a negative rule, since the positive value of life is not recognized. Human life is considered more important than property, but the life of the society comes before the life of the person. The value of life is somewhat dependent on the value of that life's service to the group. Human life is categorically higher and better than animal life. General Age 12-14 earliest; 14-16 likelier. Very Distribution: important in high school and the modal stage for the U.S. population. 102 Table 3 (continued) Stage 4% -- The Stage of Cynical Ethical Relativism Concerns and Characteristics: Same as Stage 2 except that under- standing and rejection of Stages 3 and 4 is displayed. Also, since the validity of the moral View is being questioned at this stage, the person may consciously argue for the rela- tivity of all values, claiming that terms such as "good," "bad," "right," and "wrong" should not be used at all. Stage 5 -- The Social Contract Legalistic Orientation Concerns: Characteristics: View of Justice: Safeguarding the rights of all people regardless of their socio-moral affiliation. Generating systems (both personal and in society) based on equality and formal liberty of human beings. Believes that society is designed to protect the rights of individuals. Seeks the greatest good for the greatest number. Consciously examines society's claims, but to be valid, they must reconcile with a View which everyone should take. Believes that a freely chosen "social contract" should be the basis of relationships, though once contract has been agreed to, its terms take precedence over the liberty of the contractees. Beyond the social contract, duty is considered to be a matter of personal moral choice in terms of self-chosen values. Believes that the only purposes of punishment are to: a) rehabilitate the offender, or b) maintain law and order to maintain society. 1-. ( I 1 3n AL; 103 Table 3 (continued) Stage 5 -- The Social Contract Legalistic Orientation (cont.) Concept of Rights: Value of Life: General Age Distribution: Rejects expiation and retribution, and believes that punishment is not punitive. In determining what is fair, balances: a) what is right for the individual with b) the greatest good for the greatest number. Person recognizes both: a) rights associated with status and role, and b) unearned, universal rights. Believes in a universal human right to life, regardless of the person's socio-moral affiliation. Life is also valued in terms of its contribution to community welfare. Early 20's; mid-late 20's likelier. Kohlberg believes only 15% of the U.S. population ever reaches this stage. Stage 6 -- The Universal Ethical Principle Orientation Concern: Characteristics: Equality, freedom, dignity for all persons. Sees worth of all individuals as intrinsic and universal. Sees self as part of all humanity and of history (consequences of personal actions are seen in historical per- spective). Thinks that trust is important for itself, for its role in maintaining principled relationships. 104 Table 3 (continued) Stage 6 -- The Universal Ethical Principle Orientation ' (cont.) View of Justice: Considers all claims; action to benefit the least advantaged person(s) is considered most just. Concept of Every right implies a duty. Rights: Value of Life: Life is a universal human right and a duty to save it. General Age Late 20's, early 30's if at all. Distribution: Kohlberg thinks less than 5 percent of the U.S. population reaches this stage. 105 will likely be made at various times according to the logic of each of the lower stages, though as the modal stage increases, there is a tendency for fewer and fewer judgments to be made at the lowest stages. During periods of transition between stages of values develop- ment, however, the highest stage and the modal stage will be different. In addition to structure and stage, Kohlberg postulates that five additional factors are directly involved in values development. These are: l. Egocentrism-Perspectivism, as a dimension of self and other understanding 2. Genetic emergence, organismic growth, organic maturation 3. Equilibration process and cybernetic factors 4. Experience 5. Social transmission, socialization, education. 16 Of these factors, egocentrism-perspectivism is a process linked directly to structure and stage, whereas the other four are associated with development. Experience and social transmission are factors derived from the environment; genetic emergence and equilibration are functional factors. All of these factors require some further explanation. Perspectivism, the opposite of egocentrism, refers to an increasing state of personal awareness 106 which enables the individual at each successive stage of cognitive and moral development to take the point of View of ever larger numbers of people. The newborn infant is totally egocentric, without being aware that he is. Even in the first few months of his life, however, the process that Piaget calls "de-centering" begins. Piaget writes: ... on the plane of knowledge (as, perhaps, on that of moral and aesthetic values) the subject's activity calls for a continual "de-centering" without which he cannot become free from his spontaneous intellectual ego- centricity. This "de-centering" makes the subject enter upon, not so much an already available and therefore external universality, as an uninterrupted process of coordinating and setting in reciprocal relations. 17 Piaget goes on in this passage to say that "it is the latter process, the process of reciprocal relations, which is the true 'generator' of structures as constantly under construction and reconstruction." In other words, expanding perspectivism is directly correlated to the passage from one stage to another. Kohlberg has translated Piaget's statements on egocentrism/perspectivism into a more directly quanti- fiable and progressive developmental pattern for use with 18 his moral development stages. At Stage 1, the indi- vidual's personal point of view is the only one under- stood and the only one valued. At Stage 2, the individual is able to take the perspective of the other person and 107 to view himself as others View him. The Stage 3 indi- vidual steps outside the two-person situation and achieves a third-person perspective on dyadic relation- ships. He realizes that both he and the other individual can consider each other's point of view simultaneously and mutually. The individual at Stage 4 sees things from the perspective of the public or the generalized member of society who belongs to several groups and who has developed a perspective toward that which is common to all groups; i.e., to society as a system. At Stages 5 and 6, the individual's perspective is essen- tially analytical. Aware of the relativity of individual and social group perspectives, the principled thinker (Stages 5 and 6) weighs and orders certain social data (that which is available to him) to predict his own behavior and that of others in complex social situations. Of the functional factors responsible for develop- ment, the set of factors including genetic maturation and organic growth is most closely tied to the physio- logical development of the individual's central nervous system. Genetic maturation is necessary but not suffi- cient for cognitive development, which is a necessary prerequisite to perspectivism, which in turn permits employment of the moral judgment structures at any given stage. As Piaget puts it: 108 ... this does not mean we can assume there exists a hereditary program underlying the development of human intelligence: there are no "innate ideas".... Thus the effects of maturation consist essentially of opening new possibilities for development, that is, giving access to structures which could not be evolved before these possibilities were offered. But between possibility and actual- ization, there must intervene a set of other factors such as exercise, experience, and social interaction. 19 Equilibration is closely linked to the process of equilibrium, whereby the functions of assimilation and accommodation are maintained in balance in the human intelligence, but it transcends this process as well. According to Piaget and Kohlberg, equilibration is almost synonymous with self-regulation. By a series of steps which Piaget outlines, the individual senses conflict at not understanding a particular concept or task. One dimension or approach to solution is then considered and tried, followed by others until a type of resolution occurs.20 Inherent within the concept of equilibration is the notion that at each successive stage of intellectual/ values developmenththe individual can more totally under- stand concepts andgproblems of all types. Thus resolu- tions are more "equilibrated," or satisfying to the individual. Since equilibration is related in large part to the individual's internal capacity to deal with con- flict, and since conflict seems to be a necessary pre- requisite to discovering the superiority of the logic of 109 successive stages, it is key to values development.21 The environmental factor of experience refers both to direct contact with objects and actions in the material world and to logico-mathematical experience, or 22 Kohlberg believes exposure to analytical thinking. that a cognitively and physically enriched environment, including a high level of formal education and exposure to a variety of direct physical experiences with objects in the real world, supplies the type of experience that allows for development. A less rich environment provides less opportunity for development.23 Social transmission, also a factor of the environ- ment, refers to the opportunities provided by the insti- tutions of the society for role-taking or assuming the perspectives of other pe0ple. There must be examples in the culture of individuals at higher levels of moral development so that individuals can role-play their experiences and judgments and come to understand the logic of the higher stages.24 Kohlberg's research has indicated that although the complete range of stages is to be found in the United States, most U.S. adults fixate 25 at either Stage 3 or Stage 4. Although Kohlberg states that Stage 4 is the most frequently observed or modal of 26 he thinks that our society (at least theoretically), socialization practices for women have, until very recently, been oriented toward Stage 3 of values 110 development,27 whereas men more uniformly exemplify the mode. Some cultures contain no Stage 5 or 6 individuals, or Stage 3 or 4 individuals, either, for that matter.28 Within the range of environmental factors that contribute to creating the possibility for values develop- ment, it is obvious that the mass media, and particularly television, is one institution that provides ample opportunity for role-taking. As noted above, it is role- taking that is so crucial to the development of a per- spectivist dimension of self which in turn allows moral development to proceed from stage to stage. In order for role-taking to be an effective force in values deve10pment, however, the individual must have available to him models of the higher stages. What remains uncer- tain is the level of values deve10pment portrayed by television, or any mass medium for that matter. With regard to the stages of values development portrayed by television characters, this dissertation aims to in part answer that question. Chapter 5 will describe in greater detail how the Kohlberg theory will be applied to an analysis of the values of TV characters. 111 Notes to Chapter 4 1Accounts of Kohlberg's work have thus far been limited to his dissertation (The Development of Modes of Moral Thinking and Choice in the Years Ten to Sixteen, University of Chicago, 1958) and to numerous articles on moral judgment in academic journals, some of which have been collected to form Laurence Kohlberg's Collected Papers (Laboratory for Human Development, Cambridge, 1973). Though the most detailed information on the stages is found in Kohlberg's dissertation, that information has undergone considerable reevaluation and change as a result of Kohlberg's continuing interviews of his longitudinal subjects and of his ongoing research with greater numbers of subjects. Since Kohlberg's writing fails to keep up with his research, the most up-to-date clarifications of the characteristics of the stages are currently found in the writing of his inter- preters. The information contained in the table is extracted from John S. Stewart, Values Development Education (E. Lansing, 1973), 83-103. The paragraphs under the heading "Basis of Moral Judgment" are from Kohlberg and Peter Sharf, "Bureaucratic Violence and Conventional Moral Thinking," first published in American Journal of Orthopsychiatry (April 6, 1972). 2Kohlberg, "A Cognitive-Developmental Approach to Moral Education," in The Humanist (November/December 1972). See also Stewart, Values Development Education, 100. Stewart summarizes Kohlberg's view that the Stage 6 individual must have faced "sustained responsibility for the welfare of others; irreversible moral decisions in actual life situations; and high level cognitive stimulation, conflict, and reflection." 3Stewart, Toward a Theory for Values Development Education, Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1974, p. 173. 4In addition to Piaget, Kohlberg and Stewart, other prOponents of the "organismic" view and their pertinent works include: a. John Dewey, "The Unity of the Human Being," (a 1937 speech to the College of Physicians) in J. Ratner, Intelligence in the Modern World: John Deweyig Philosophy, New York, 1939. John Dewey, Theory of the Moral Life, New York, 1960 (originally published as Ethics in 1932, with co-author J.H. Tufts). 112 b. Jan Christian Smutz, Holism and Evaluation, New York, 1926. c. H. Werner, "The Concept of Development from a Comparative and Organismic Point of View," in D. Harris, Ed., The Concept of Development, Minneapolis, 1957. H. Werner and B. Kaplan, Symbol Formation: An Organismic-Developmental Approach to Language and the Expression of Thought, New York, 1963. d. K. Goldstein, The Organism, New York, 1939. e. Abraham Maslow, Toward a Psychology of Being, New York, 1968. 5Stewart outlines these three features of the organismic view in Values Development Education, 17-19. 6Steward, Toward a Theory for Values Development Education, 185. 7Piaget's theory has been widely published and is a primary theme reappearing throughout his many works on psychology, epistemology, education and child development. The bases of the theory are conveniently available in Jean Piaget, "Piaget's Theory," P. Mussen, ed., Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology, 3rd. ed. (New York, 1970), 703-732. 8Piaget's seminal works include: a. The Language and Thought of the Child (New York, 1926). b. Judgment and Reasoning in the Child (New York, 1928). c. The Child's Conception of the World (New York, 1929). d. The Origins of Intelligence in Children (New York, 1952). 9Table 2 is a condensation of John Stewart's Tables 4.1A through 4.1D in Toward a Theory of Values Development Education, 282-330. loSteward, Values Development Education, 65. 11L. Kohlberg and staff, Standard Scoring Manual, Form A-l (Laboratory for Human Development, Harvard University, Cambridge, 1974), 4. 113 lzIbid., 4-5. l3Stewart, Values Development Education, 65. l4Condensed from Stewart, Toward a Theory, 326-372. Kohlberg acknowledges that Stewart's dis- sertation (1974) makes him one of Kohlberg's most up-to- date interpreters. The Values Development Education Program at Michigan State University is one of two centers (in addition to the Laboratory for Human Deve10pment at Harvard, where Kohlberg is Director) which is actively working toward practical applications of the Kohlberg theory. As such, one of Stewart's concerns as co-director of the Program, has been to compile, classify and simplify Kohlberg's various pronouncements on stage characteristics. For this reason, his work is especially helpful when compiling concise summaries is called for. Such summaries, however, are no substitute for readings directly from Kohlberg (the collected papers are especially helpful) and for first-hand practice with the scoring manual, and they are not presented as such here. 15Stewart, Values Development Education, 78. 16Stewart, Toward a Theory, 299. 17Jean Piaget, Structuralism, New York, 1970, 139. 18See Stewart, Values Development Education, 37-42; also 83-103. Also see Robert Seeman,"The Relation of Role-Taking to the Development of Moral Judgment in Children," in Child Development, 42 (1971), 79-92. (Selman is an associate of Kohlberg's at Harvard.) Kohlberg's piece, "Moral Development," written for the International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences (New York, 1968), 489-494, and reprinted in Kohlberg's Collected ngers, is also helpful. 19Piaget, "Piaget's Theory," 719-720. 2°Ibid., 725. 21See Stewart, Toward a Theory, 308-312. 22"Piaget's Theory," 721. 15. 114 23Stewart, Toward a Theory, 302-306. 24Ibid., 306-308. 25Kohlberg, "A Cognitive-Developmental Approach," 26Stewart, Values Development Education, 91. 27Ibid., 89. 28 Ibid., 68-69. See also Kohlberg, "Stage and Sequence" in D. Goslin, Ed., Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research (New York, 1969). CHAPTER 5 METHODOLOGY FOR APPLYING KOHLBERG'S THEORY TO AN ANALYSIS OF TELEVISION CHARACTERS The previous chapter has explained Lawrence Kohlberg's Theory of Values Development in some detail, but it remains to be shown how the concepts generated by the theory were applied to an analysis of the values of television characters. Section I of the present chapter will therefore describe the procedure by which particular programs and characters were selected for analysis. Section II will then describe the technique of analysis used. This description will consist of: l) a demonstration based on the scoring technique used by Kohlberg to determine the values of actual subjects, and 2) an explanation of how this process was applied to television characters, including an example of a TV character at each stage of values development. Section I: Selection of Programs and Characters Characters were chosen for study on the basis of the popularity of the programs in which they appeared. Primarily, the measure used to assess pOpularity was the size of the viewing audience in any given season, based on Nielsen ratings. The primary interest of this study is to make a comparison, over a specific period of time, of the values of television characters, based on 115 116 sex, age, occupation and on the type of program in which the character appeared. I used the most pOpular pro- grams since: a) they reach the greatest number of people, and a subsidiary goal of this research is to compare the values of televison charac- ters to those of the viewing public, accor- ding to Kohlberg's prognosis, and b) they tend to portray characters with values most representative of those portrayed on television dramatic-type programs as a whole. It is possible to assume that characters on popular programs are representative of TV characters in general because of the desire for a secure profit on the part of network officials, discussed in Chapter 3. The fact is that popular television programs tend to be continued for a number of seasons, and they are widely imitated.l Some of the imitations are in turn popular, and some only ride the bandwagon, so to speak, since they never achieve better than mediocre ratings and are discon- tinued after a season or two. In combination, however, even these mediocre imitators represent a substantial part of the TV program diet at any given time. Ratings published by the A. C. Nielsen tele- vision rating service were used to measure popularity of programs. Technically, the Nielsen reports are confi- dential, released only to those who subscribe to the service, although summary ratings are regularly released to trade publications by network officials and others. 117 Legitimate subscribers include large commercial busi- nesses, advertising agencies and occasionally indepen- dent purchasers, as well as the networks themselves.2 Summary Nielsen's, as they appear in trade publications, typically give both a "rating" and a "share“ for each program. The "rating" represents, in millions, the total number of households which, on an average basis, viewed the program during a given report period. As an example, top-rated All in the Family received an average rating of 29.4 for the period September 9 through October 20, 1974. This means that, on an average weekly basis, 29.4 million families watched the program. The "share“ equals the percent of the total tuned-in viewing audience that the program captured. In All in the Family's case, the share for the first half of the 1974-75 season was 51, or 51% of the total audience watching television between 8:00 and 8:30 on Saturday nights. Nielsen ratings for this study were taken from Variety, a weekly trade magazine of the entertainment industries. Although there was no particular schedule on which Nielsen ratings appeared in Variety, a "tOp-ZO" or "top-30" listing generally appeared at least once a month during the main viewing season, beginning with October and ending with May. Articles analyzing or further summarizing the ratings as well as special 118 ratings for particular cities or for rerun editions of programs also appeared on an unsystematic but fairly regular basis. Since it was necessary to page through individual issues of the magazine in order to locate ratings, I focused on the months of October and Novem- ber both for the sake of convenience and to gain some consistency over the years covered. These particular months seemed desirable choices because the excitement generated by the new season and the resulting interest in the new ratings led to the publication of a larger number of lists and articles than appeared in other months. After locating a summary Nielsen, however, I checked at least one subsequent list to make sure that special programming had not pre-empted or down-rated normally pOpular programs. I considered this to be a particularly likely occurrence in national election years, but it turned out to be of fairly minimal impor- tance. In general, as Table 6 below indicates, the most popular programs not only tended to be ranked consis- tently high during the season, but they also tended to thrive for several seasons. Selecting Current Season Programs The prime focus of the present study is the fall, 1974 viewing season. Accordingly, the seven most pOpu- lar programs in each category were selected for analysis, 119 with the exception that there are only five programs in the drama category for this season, and all of them were included. This seemed a reasonable number of programs to cover in the time period employed. Titles chosen for the 1974 season analysis, together with their Nielsen ranks and ratings, appear in Table 4. It should be noted that with the exception of the two lower—ranked drama programs (including previously tOp-ranked Marcus Welby, M.D.) all of these programs received Nielsen ratings of 20.0 or above. Television critics consider this a safe range of popularity, guaranteeing network interest in continuing the program. The ranks and ratings for all prime-time programs aired during the fall, 1974 season are presented in Table 5 for purposes of comparison. SelectingPast Programs In addition to the 1974 season analysis, I wanted to examine past programs beginning with the year 1960 to determine what, if any, changes in characters' values have occurred during the past fifteen years. It is well known that during this period of American his- tory dramatic social changes took place, including the beginnings of a new era of American feminism which resulted in renewed evaluations of male and female roles in this country. Accordingly, I was especially interes- ted to discover what, if any, changes have taken place 120 TABLE 4 1974-75 Season Programs Selected for Analysis, by Nielsen Rank and Nielson Rating Program Rank Rating Category PrOgram Title (Nielsen)* (Nielsen)* Situation All in the Family 1 29.4 Comedy Sanford and Son 2 28.7 Chico and the Man 3 28.4 Rhoda 4 26.6 M*A*S*H* 6 25.2 Maude 7 24.8 Mary Tyler Moore Show 8(tie) 23.6 Drama The Waltons 5 25.6 Little House on the Prairie 8(tie) 23.6 Medical Center 17(tie) 21.7 Lucas Tanner 39 18.1 Marcus Welby, M.D. 44 17.5 Drama- Hawaii Five-O 13(tie) 22.4 Adventure Streets of San Francisco 15 22.0 Rockford Files 17(tie) 21.7 Kojak 19 21.6 Rookies 20 21.0 Gunsmoke 21 20.9 Police Woman 25 20.2 *From Variet , 30 October 74, p. 57. Ratings are for the period September 9 through October 20, 1974. 121 TABLE 5 Programs in the Fall 1974 Television Season* by Rating and Program Category Proqram Rank Program Title Category** Rating 1 All in the Family SC 29.4 2 Sanford and Son SC 28.7 3 Chico and the Man SC 28.4 4 Rhoda SC 26.6 5 The Waltons D 25.6 6 M*A#S*H* SC 25.2 7 Maude SC 24.8 8 Little House on the Prairie D 23.6 Mary Tyler Moore Show SC 23.6 10 Good Times SC 23.4 11 Disney V 23.2 12 Movie*** - 22.9 13 Bob Newhart Show SC 22.4 Hawaii Five-O DA 22.4 15 Streets of San Francisco DA 22.0 16 Friends and Lovers SC 21.8 17 Medical Center D 21.7 Rockford Files DA 21.7 19 Kojak DA 21.6 20 Rookies DA 21.0 21 Gunsmoke DA 20.9 22 Movie 20.7 23 Movie 20.7 24 Movie 20.4 25 Police Woman DA 20.2 26 NFL Football 19.9 The Manhunter DA 19.9 28 Emergency DA 19.8 That's My Mama SC 19.8 Movie 19.8 31 Police Story DA 19.7 Movie 19.7 33 Carol Burnett Show V 19.4 Mannix DA 19.4 35 Movie 19.2 36 Cannon DA 19.1 37 Movie 18.9 38 Movie 18.2 39 Lucas Tanner D 18.1 40 Happy Days SC 17.9 41 Apple's Way D 17.9 122 Table 5 (continued) Program Rank Program Title Category** Rating 42 Barnaby Jones DA 17.6 Born Free 17.6 44 Marcus Welby, M.D. D 17.5 45 Harry O DA 17.2 Movin On 17.2 47 Get Christie Love DA 16.8 48 Adam-12 DA 16.2 49 Planet of the Apes DA 16.1 Petrocelli DA 16.1 51 Sons and Daughters SC 15.6 52 Paper Moon SC 15.1 53 Sierra 15.0 54 Ironside DA 14.4 Sonny Comedy Revue V 14.4 56 Odd Couple SC 14.2 57 Nakia DA 12.7 58 Six Million Dollar Man DA 12.1 59 Night Stalker DA 12.0 60 Kung Fu DA 11.9 61 Texas Wheelers 11.0 62 Kodiak 9.9 63 The New Land 7.9 *Ratings are for the period September 9 through October 20, 1974. From Variety, 30 October 74, p. 57. **SC = Situation Comedy; D = Drama; DA = Drama/Adventure; V = Variety. ***Movies had specific titles (such as NBC Friday Night Movies) but these were omitted since they are irrelevant to the present study. 123 in recent years regarding the values of male and female television characters. Like 1974 season programs, past programs were also selected primarily on the basis of popularity. For each year between 1960 and 1973, the tOp five to seven shown in each program category (sitcom, drama, drama/ adventure) were selected from Nielsen's list of "top twenty." The selected programs were then charted (Table 6) and a composite list for each category was drawn up (Table 7). Finally, the availability of the programs, either in rerun or in the Michigan State University script collection, was ascertained. As Table 4 shows, this yielded: ten Situation comedies, spanning the years 1960-1974; ten drama-adventure programs, also spanning the years 1960-1974, and one drama program. The selection of past programs presented several logistical problems not associated with the selection of 1974 season programs. First, both reruns and scripts are in limited distribution at the present time. For- tunately, many of the most popular past programs, mea- sured both by Nielsen ratings and length of original run, are currently available in rerun or script editions. Number of Programs Viewed A minimum of ten per cent of the episodes (or appearances per principle character, whichever was 124 Ammv mnfl£3mm mesons magma «eaaamv cowuocsn umooauumm names comes a auauuauo “one monmcso ammo com 305m 09A smacwmufl> oumcawx .Ho oxho sm> xowo “.900 one ancmcom comm: waned mafiaaunaawm aauo>om mood Hmumm mesa cm> gown auaumeuo send A.umz. camps comm: sown: muumm moeona scene A V oxOEmsso mumcawx .uo damn maaaosq .>oz h mucmcom ammmo com mowaaanflawm maum>mm Nmma am>mu9 Hawz .csu m>mm muwocmmmo em HMO Henna mxosmcso meow mouse a: mucmcom whoop: Hmmm mesons magma A.>oz my names comes down: muumm spammwuo hogs Homa dwuum ummssm sh A.um V mesons assoc mwabmnosouso «« z oomsww on» madame Hm>mua «4A.umzv m undo a mumm Has: .:50 m>mm ummm mzosx Hmnumm mccsm do magmawum gamma comma sown: muumm acom m as A.>oz mo exosmaso mucous Hmmm nuemmauo mega omma muommumo musucm>o¢\msmno manna homeou Emumoum use» «whomwumo Emumoum an .mnmanowma How msmumoum emuMMIcmmewz mos m mdmda 125 HHmuxmo mcom mmHne a: mcoom amazon mmHom :mme umeMHo Hammad SHflEmm moprGOHH cmnouflswm monmsso dammed 3onm mood smachHH> momHm acumen mahm HoEow A.uoo mmv mncmcom com maucmw HHHMMHHQ mead noma uHmEm How Hem H emeoeesmm cchHmHH> mmHo< somHo HOHHmm Ham mowaawbaafim MHHm>mm HHmuxmo mung A.>oz mv mesmeom euHmmHHo Home momH mmOHmm m.smmom GOHHossh umOOHHuwm HHmEm H00 meoom Hchmo nuHHHHHo Heed mqozs ECHm cm: >054 smachHH> mama HmEoo A.>oz oav cmsmcom mmfiaawnaafim mHHm>mm moma QHHMMHHO mood mama H0500 smwuHmz mHHHo>mm >2 mxmo cm> xowo mxoamsso Gomez mHHmm GOHHUGSH Hooveuumm Hansoo HH moMHd doused x.n0mc m>wuflmsm H momam coummm mowaaanaaflm >HHO>mm A.>oz Hay ousmsom dammed cmnouHBmm wood mHsucm>©¢\mEMHo MEMHQ mcmaoo wmmwwwwm Hmmw Acmscwusoov m OHDMB ...— 126 ««A.>ozv ummHummsoq nusmsom wucsoo m.oomu on0>flm me3mm 303m moo mHHoo desoo eeo mxmo cm> gown 00mm Madam mHuEmem mmm zoom mHHemm mooHHuHmm waEmcso Housmu amoecmz aosq m.mHmm A.uoo may Hedges .o.z .anmz meonz HHHsmm 0:» eH HHe HemH NHIEmcd mawewm moowHuHmm mofichHH Hmnumm m.mflccm cucmcom mmEonB wanna cmsvm 00: 30am mac mHHoo ouo>flm Hamsmm mmm Eoom Hammmfi maflamm .H.m.m Hmucmo HmoHomz .o.m.m HHHmnsmz x.>oz HHV oonmcso .o.z .mnamz mSOHmS moan onma >054 m.mHmm NHIEnpm pmnopw3mm cmsvm cos modaawnaaflm >HHm>om mcwm:0HH mcom m a: .H.m.m Hmuamu amoecoz 30pm use me00 oucmsom mmm Boom .o.m.m mHHmnmmz A.Hoo NNV meEmcsw .o.z .mQHmS mDonz Hammmd SHHEmm moma cMHsHmHH> mmHHHHnHHHm >HH0>mm maoom amazon >094 m.mHmm meEmsso Hammmd maflemm manflmmomEH seammwz cease meHmeoHH mHmd Hmsoo A.uoo MNV mucmsom .o.m.m mHHmnmmz mead aHommumo mHsusm>p¢\mEMHo mEMHQ momfioo EMHGOHm Ham» AemscHucooc e deems 127 .mOHHHHo >9 ma mmHMH muHHnasmom emmmme m :w on on cmHmcwmsoo mHm om m>onm mstGMH mEMHmOHm mocwm .sm>flm mHm om onmQ mmcHxCMH Mace .umHH somamwz may no EMHmOHm on» no xGMH on» o» mHmmmH HmQEdz««* .maosz m we How» on» Ho“ csonm been one» Hmnuo mcHxGMH Manusos m EOHM cocoa EMHmOHQ <«« .Hmmm some How szonm sumo map so msflummmfi NumHHm> cw chHHmEESm mm .mmsfiumH comamfiz eom mow: Ho eom mou: EOHM mEmo mEMHmOHm« Sosa m.mHmm mocOb mnmchm 3onw uHmnsmz nom mmeoom .141.Hd¢ .mmo 30am xmmom Housmo Hecate: mHooz Hmaha MHME «finance A.Hmdv mmz m.mamm¢ moss: oHOEmcso «eA.Hm¢v «m«m«¢«z coacmo anamz mDOHmz now can UHomcmm A.omo mHv oum>Hm HHmsmm mcouHms was magsmm 0a» :H Ham memH mmflxoom 305m moo mHHoo manages 303m mHooz Hwama mHmz mUHmGOHH mHsHmm mm>oq Homome oxoamasw mcsmz cocamo moon m.mHom cum>wm me3mm com can oHomsmm A.>oz mv stcmz .o.z .mnamz msonS MHHEmm on» Ca HH< mnma MHommumo mHsucm>U4\mEMHo 8EMHQ MpwEoo EMHmOHm How» Aedsqeueooo e 0Han 128 TABLE 7 Composite Listing of Top Nielsen-Rated Programs, 1960-1973, by Program Type, and Availability of Reruns or Scripts Program Program Title Availability of Category Reruns or Scripts Situation Danny Thomas Comedy Dennis the Menace My Three Sons Hazel Andy Griffith In rerun Beverly Hillbillies In rerun Lucy Show In rerun Dick Van Dyke Show In rerun Gomer Pyle In rerun Get Smart Petticoat Junction In rerun Hogan's Heroes In rerun Green Acres Here's Lucy Bewitched In rerun Family Affair In rerun Mayberry R.F.D. ' Julia Doris Day Show Eddie's Father Partirdge Family In rerun All in the Family Funny Face New Dick Van Dyke Show Odd Couple Sanford and Son Maude Bridget Loves Bernie Mary Tyler Moore Show M*A*S*H* Bob Newhart Show Drama Father Knows Best Real McCoys Perry Mason Ben Casey Dr. Kildare Lassie Gentle Ben Peyton Place 129 Table 7 (continued) Program Category Program Title Availability of Reruns or Scripts Drama Drama/ Adventure Marcus Welby, M.D. Room 222 Medical Center The Waltons Apple's Way Gunsmoke Wagon Train Have Gun, Will Travel Bonanza Car 54 Defenders Virginian Man from UNCLE Daniel Boone Daktari Rat Patrol I SPY Drganet Ironsides Mission Impossible F.B.I. Mod Squad Adam-12 Hawaii Five-O Mannix Cade's County Longstreet Owen Marshall Cannon Tookies Kojak Barnaby Jones In rerun Script-l Scripts-3 Scripts-2 In rerun Scripts-6 Scripts-2 In rerun Scripts-2 Scripts-2 Script-l 130 greater) were viewed for the 1974 season analysis. The limited number of scripts both at the Michigan State University Library's Special Collections Department and in distribution in general, necessitated using a fewer number of examples per program than was desired. In some cases, only one script per program was available, though more frequently, there were at least two. To maintain a level of generality consistent with the availability of scripts, a maximum of three programs (or appearances per principal character) were read or viewed for the historical review section. Although a more thorough analysis of past season programs would be desirable as more data become avail- able, I considered the present historical analysis sufficient for the present study since: 1) the focus for this study is on the 1974 season, 2) the programs available for the historical review cover the selected time period, 1960-1974, in the two major categories of programming-—situation comedy and drama-adventure, and 3) time constraints based on the appropriate completion period for a dissertation study would have severely limited the amount of additional analysis that the pres- ent study could have incorporated, even if additional data had been available. 4. 131 Section II: Technique of Analysis As each episode was viewed (or read), a one- or two-page analysis was drawn up which contained 1) a statement of plot, and 2) a listing of the major charac- ters, both regular and non-regular, together with any dialogue which revealed their rationales for taking/not taking or approving/not approving viewpoints or courses of action posed as possibilities by the plot. At the conclusion of each episode, a stage or stages of values deve10pment was assigned to each character. Then, after the pre-determined number of viewings-per-principal- character were completed, the stages gleaned from the separate viewings were compared and the highest stage portrayed at least twice was selected as a given charac- ter's most commonly portrayed stage of values deve10p- ment. It should be emphasized that the most important "technique" involved in conducting the present analysis was a thorough and ready understanding of the Kohlberg stages, especially including the ability to differen- tiate between "content" and "structure" as these con- cepts are defined in the previous chapter. It is my belief that this ability comes only as a result of extensive reading in the theoretical literature on Kohlberg's stages. In addition, however, practice in applying the stage concept is also necessary, and the 132 Kohlberg staff has published the Standard Scoring Manual to help meet this need.3 The Scoring Manual contains six dilemmas of the type quoted in Chapter 4, together with the responses of a large number of actual subjects to questions posed about these dilemmas. The subjects' responses are categorized according to stage of values development, and a brief eXplanatory sentence or two is provided to further help the amateur scorer understand why the logic of a subject's answers fits a particular stage. In an attempt to explain more fully the process used to determine the values of television characters, I will first demonstrate how a standard Kohlberg “dilemma" would be scored, using examples taken from the manual. In doing this, I will show how content is differentiated from structure at each stage. Following this explanation based on the Kohlberg scoring procedure, I will show more specifically how the process was applied to television characters by giving an example of a character at each stage of values deve10pment and showing how that character's stage level was determined. Recall from Chapter 4 the example of the dilemma involving Joe and his father. Joe is a 14-year-old boy who wanted to go to camp very much. His father promised him he could go if he saved up the money for it himself. 133 So Joe worked hard at his paper route and saved up the $40 it cost to go to camp and a little more besides. But just before camp was going to start, his father changed his mind. Some of his friends decided to go on a special fishing trip, and Joe's father was short of the money it would cost. So he told Joe to give him the money he had saved from the paper route. Joe didn't want to give up going to camp, so he thought of refusing to give his father the money. Now, also recall that one of the two issues that this dilemma is designed to test is the subject's view of the basis of the father-son relationship.* In brief, for each stage, the viewpoints are:4 Stage l--Authority of the father. Obedience of the son. Stage 2--Reciprocity, exchange and instrumental need. Stage 3--Mutua1 trust, understanding, concern, respect and affection. *In "Moral Development and Moral Education," in Kohlberg's Collected Papers on Moral Develo ment and Moral Education, Kohlberg and associate EIIiot Turiel set forth eleven "Issues, Institutions, or Norms," which are basic to moral judgment and from.which the issues chosen for various dilemmas seem to be derived. These include: 1. Social rules 2. The self, the ideal self, and conscience 3. Helping roles, involving c00peration, help- fulness, and affection (special obliga- tions to role-partners) 4. Authority issues and roles, governmental roles 5. Civil liberties 6. Contract, promise, and reciprocity 7. Punitive justice 8. Life as a value and a right 9. Property 10. Truth 11. Self and sexual love 134 Stage 4--Defined responsibilities of a father and a son. Stage 5--Recognition of each as free and equal individuals whose rights are deserving of equal respect. Stage 6--Not scored for, since so few individ- uals ever reach this stage. The following are examples of responses to one of the questions designed to elicit the subject's view of the father-son relationship. Though the scoring manual contains several sample responses for each stage of development, only one or two per stage are presented here. QUESTION: Stage 1 Answer: Stage 2 Subject 1 Subject 2 SHOULD JOE REFUSE TO GIVE HIS FATHER THE MONEY? WHY? No, because that's his son and if he doesn't give it to him, he would get punished. And he is the head person in the house and tells his children what to do. He has to do what his father sgys or he will get a lickin'. No, because his father has paid plenty for Joe. His father pays for the food he eats and the room he sleeps in. And even if Joe did earn the money gimself, he should give it to him. Yes, because it's his money, some- . thing he saved for. He had to save for his own trip. His father should save up for his. Stage 3 Subject 1: Subject 2: 135 No, if Joe loved his father he would realize how much his father has done for him, and that he could give his father the $40. He could always go to camp, but maybe8this was a spe- cial fishing trip. Yes, because it wasn't something really necessary that his father did it for, it was just some selfish desire of his father's. And since his father had said he could go before, he should stick to big word to keep the trust of the son. Stage 3-A Prime Answer: Stage 4 Subject 1: Subject 2: No, because parents' word is law and we are supposed to obey and as far as we arelable, we should avoid disobedience. No, I think a child has a primary responsibility of obedience to a parent. From this point of view, I don't think there is any question that Joe should if what his father tells him to do. Yes, because Joe saved up the money, right?... I don't think a son has the responsibility to take care of his father in that sort of situation. Just because he's the guy's son doesn't mean the father can take ad- vantage of him by trying to get his money for what he wants. If it were a situation like, where somebody's life were at stake, or health, I think he would have the responsibili ity, but not for a pleasure thing. 136 Stage 4 1/2* Subject 1: Not if he wants to be practical about it. I mean, it's his decision and nobody can say what he should or shouldn't do, but life won't be a bed of roses for a l4-year-old kid who defies his father. Subject 2: Yes, if he feels like it. After all, the kid's got his rights too. He worked for the money, so what he does with it is up to him. Stage 5 Subject 1: (No.) The situation is so unfair, that Joe would not have an obliga- tion. I think the situation is unfair and I think the rules are not established, and you therefore would not have an obligation for any reason except the maturity of the father. I think, though, the answer is to give it to him, but not because of any obligation. The answer is in terms of long-term bestness. Today I will be unhappy, but later we will come back and maybe there will be some balance.1 Subject 2: (Yes.) Children do not owe parents absolute obedience, either in posi- tive or restrictive duties. A parent who acts unfairly toward a child has lost ground for commanding respect.14 The examples above were chosen with the specific goal in mind of illustrating the difference between content and structure in reasoning about values. Note *These are my hypothetical examples since the manual does not score specifically for Stage 4 1/2. 137 that for each stage with the exceptions of Stage 1 and Stage 3-A Prime, the sample responses are opposed in content. That is, one subject thinks Joe should give his father the money and the other subject, also at that particular stage of values development, thinks Joe should not give his father the money. But both examples of each stage reflect the structure of rea— soning of that stage as it applies to the issue of the basis of the father-son relationship. In the case of Stage 1, there is only one possible content of the response to the question since the Stage 1 individual, concerned about the consequences of disobeying an authority figure, can think of no reasons for refusing to give the father the money. In other words, the structure of reasoning dictates only one possible content: give the father the money. In the case of Stage 2, the reasoning structure is oriented to reciprocity, exchange of favors and instrumental need. This can lead the Stage 2 indi- vidual to think of the father and son as the principals involved in this exchange and reciprocity, but it can just as easily lead to a view of pgpp father and son as pe0ple with the right to meet their instrumental needs. (“Needs" here translates to mean "desires.") Subject one takes the former position, deciding that Joe should give (not refuse) his father the money in 138 exchange for the room and board the father has provided him in the past--and no doubt will provide for him in the future. Subject two, on the other hand, focuses on work as a type of exchange in which one performs certain duties and then has a right to complete hedon- istic control of the money gained by doing this work. Since Joe is the one who worked for the money, subject two reasons that it is his to spend; the father should work for his own money. Stage 3 inidividuals see the basis of the father- son relationship as consisting primarily of trust, under- standing, concern, affection and so forth. These traits of relationships, however, are not visible in the same manner as instrumental rewards, and their legitimacy depends in large part on the inner motives or "inten- tions" of the individuals involved. Accordingly, the Stage 3 subjects place a lot of emphasis on the inten- tions involved when considering the situation between Joe and his dad. Subject one assumes that Joe's father is benevolent and has good intentions (it is common for Stage 3 peOple to assume that authority figures are benevolent). On this basis, he feels that Joe should give his father the money, since the presumption is that love is a two-way street between parent and child and that Joe's father would not ask for the money if this truly weren't a special fishing trip. Subject two, 139 however, views the motives Of Joe's father as "selfish desire," and on this basis thinks Joe is justified in refusing to give him the money. Even more than this, though, subject two brings up the issue of "trust," recognizing that Joe's father is violating his close personal relationship with his son by demanding the money for spurious reasons. The response which I have classified as 3-A Prime (it appears just as 3A in the manual) shows a concern for maintaining the arbitrary roles of father and son: "parents' word is law and we are supposed to Obey." As Opposed to Stage 1, however, fear of auto- matic punishment is not the reason for obedience. Neither is hOpe of instrumental reward, or exchange Of favors, as was the case with the Stage 2 subjects. Maintenance of the parent-child relationship for its own sake is clearly the issue here, but the bonds of mutual love and support are blurred by a concern for maintain- ing rank, so to speak. This concern with rank, or with following the orders of a designated superior, are Stage 3-A Prime concerns (see also Table 3, Chapter 4). The qualities Of "respect" for the earned position of the authority figure and Of "duty" to the system that both individuals are part Of (in this case, the family) are absent, however. 140 At Stage 4, the structure of reasoning switches to the duties or responsibilities which define relation- ships Of all sorts and which provide the basis for close affectional ties. Subject one thinks Joe should not refuse to give his father the money since he believes that "a child has a primary responsibility Of obedience to a parent." This is very different from saying, as the Stage 3-A Prime subject does, that "parents' word is law and we are supposed to obey." Kohlberg has learned that especially in the upper stages, the sub- ject's use of certain terms is a crucial indicator Of stage deve10pment. In the case of Stage 4, “responsi- bility" and "duty" are two such terms. Subject two also thinks in terms Of responsibilities between parent and child, but his interpretation Of this particular situation leads him to feel that Joe should refuse his father the money. “If it were a situation like, where somebody's life were at stake, or health," subject two reasons, "I think he would have the responsibility, but not for a pleasure thing." As described in the previous chapter, Stage 4 1/2 is not a true stage, since not everyone passes through it on the way to principled thinking (Stages 5 and 6). Kohlberg now believes, however, that Stage 4 1/2 does represent a structural advance over Stage 4, since subjects at this latter stage understand and 141 equate "morality" with Stage 4 thinking.15 They go on to question the validity of duty-oriented or conscien- tious morality, however, and for some period of time, adopt a mode of reasoning very close in content to Stage 2 thinking. That is, Stage 4 1/2 individuals consciously adopt a situational, relativistic ethic, but this is very different from Stage 2 thinkers, who cannot conceive Of society's point of view, or Of any claims, for that matter, beyond the immediate situation. Again, Kohlberg has found that a subject's use of certain words provides the key to distinguishing Stage 4 1/2 philOSOphical egoism from Stage 2 naive egoism. He has learned that Stage 4 1/2 subjects res- pond at higher levels of abstraction, discourse and reflectivity.l6 In the hypothetical examples given above, the content Of the Stage 4 1/2 responses is very close to the content of the Stage 2 responses Of actual subjects. Subject one in both cases thinks that Joe should give his father the money since Joe is dependent on his father for support, but the Stage 4 1/2 subject sees this as a matter Of practicality or expediency in the given situation, whereas the Stage 2 subject thinks in terms Of a genuine exchange Of favors. Also, the hypothetical Stage 4 1/2 subject is quoted as saying that "nobody can say what he should or shouldn't do," a 142 comment which clues the scorer to the conscious rela- tivism Of the Stage 4 1/2 subject. Likewise, subject two in both cases thinks that Joe should keep the money since he worked for it and therefore it is his to spend as he wishes. As Opposed to the naive and absolute instrumentalism of the Stage 2 subject, however, the Stage 4 1/2 subject recognizes that there are conflicting claims. He says that Joe should give his father the money "if he feels like it," noting that Joe has "clearly got his rights too" (in addition to those Of his father). Again, the use of the term "rights" in this context, like subject one's use of the word "practical," provides an additional clue to the scorer that the level Of thought is more SOphisticated than Stage 2 thinking. TO confirm this, Of course, the person giving the interview would ask additional ques- tions to confirm that the Stage 4 1/2 subjects do indeed understand Stage 4 reasoning, although they reject it. At Stage 5, the individual has also rejected conventional or Stage 3 and 4 morality, but he has main- tained a view that relationships of all sorts should be governed by a set of principles (different from rules, laws, or norms) that take into account such things as equality and the rights, freedom and dignity of all peOple (not just those in this society, by the way). As such, the Stage 5 or principled thinker views the basis 143 of the father-son relationship as mutual recognition that both are individuals whose rights are deserving of equal respect. Even so, Stage 5 thinkers can disagree on the content Of any decision, though the principles they use in making the decision are the same. Subject one above recognizes that Joe's father has been "so unfair" that the normal obligations of a son have lost” their validity in this situation. He nonetheless thinks that "the answer is to give it (the money) to him, but not because of any Obligation." According to subject one, "the answer is in terms of long-term bestness" for both parties. Subject two thinks that Joe's father has forfeited his legitimate grounds for the respect Of his son by acting unfairly and that Joe is now under no Obligation to Obey his father. Clearly both Stage 5 thinkers have moved beyond restrictive role stereotyping Of fathers and sons and see Joe and his father as fully equal, each with rights deserving of respect by the other. Since in practice Kohlberg recognizes that subjects think according to the logic Of more than one stage of values-deve10pment at a given time, his scoring procedure is designed to note when different stages con- verge and to indicate which stage seems to be dominant in the subject's thinking. In the case Of the dilemma involving Joe and his father, related above, there are a 144 number of different questions designed to arrive at the subject's values. Theoretically, it would be possible for a subject to answer all questions in terms of a specific stage, such as Stage 3. In practice, however, suchaisubject would more likely answer a majority (but not all) of the questions in Stage 3 terms, answering the remaining questions in terms Of one or both of the adjacent stages, in this case, Stages 2 and 4. When the latter type of mixed reSponse occurs, Kohlberg and his staff record the dominant stage first, followed by the secondary stages, which are placed in parentheses, as for instance, Stage 3(2). If a subject responds with an equal number of answers at each of two different stages, his values are recorded by writing the two stages, highest first, with a dash between, as 3-2. Having described in greater detail how Kohlberg distinguishes between content and structure, using the structure Of a subject's reasoning in order to deter- mine his level Of values deve10pment, it is now possible to explain how the values of televison characters were determined, again using their structure Of reasoning as the measure of their stage level. To do this, an example of a 1974-75 season character will be presented for each stage of values deve10pment, together with an explanation Of that character's structure of reasoning. (.9‘“ ' 145 Perhaps fortunately, no adult character in the 1974 season, prime-time programming reasons primarily at Stage 1 of values development.* Examples of this stage Of development do exist, however, in some of the jdugments made by Jim Bob Walton, youngest son on drama leader The Waltons. In one episode, Jim Bob and a friend get caught playing sleight Of hand tricks with the merchandise in Ike Godsey's general store. Ike interprets their activities as stealing and tells Jim Bob to go home and tell his father about the inci- dent. Even before entering the store, Jim Bob had been afraid of potential consequences, but he tells his dad that it was only a game and all would have been well "if I just hadn't got caught." In another episode, Jim Bob takes a chicken leg from a batch that has been prepared for a wedding dinner to be held at the Walton home. He climbs up into the tree house to eat in secret, but Older brothers Jason and Ben are already there. They convince him to return and get pieces for them as well, but Jim Bob warns them that if he gets caught, he'll say they put him up to it. Here again, Jim Bob is thinking about the consequences Of his actions and revealing a naive view Of responsibility as well, since he belives *This has not always been the case; see Chapter 7. 146 he can pass the consequences on to the authorizing party, thereby exonerating his own involvement. Unlike Stage 1, Stage 2 is a commonly found level of values development among television characters, particularly on situation comedy programs. The most popular example Of a Stage 2 characterization is All in the Family's Archie Bunker, who will be discussed at length in the following chapter. For purpose of a brief illustration, however, Fred Sanford Of number two rated sitcom Sanford and Son, serves just as well. Sanford is a junk dealer supported financially by his grown son Lamont, who lives with his father. Not only is Lamont the breadwinner; he also does all the cooking, household chores and what cleaning he can manage before Sanford clutters up the premises again. In addition, Sanford is constantly spending Lamont's meager savings on bogus get-rich-quick schemes. In other words, though good natured and charmingly eccentric, Sanford is a leech upon his son. In line after comic line, Sanford reveals that he views the father-son relationship in terms of exchange Of favors, but that he thinks that his part of the bar- gain was paid up long ago. In one episode, Sanford "buys" a prize fighter with Lamont's savings and then expects Lamont to be his sparring partner to impress a potential manager. In another, Lamont goes to a 147 psychiatrist for a cure for a headache he's had for weeks. When Sanford discovers that the psychiatrist told Lamont that his problem is that he hates his father, Sanford goes into a comic fit of depression. "After all I done for that boy,“ he says. "Why, when he's just a little baby and he'd cry out at night 'cause he needed somethin', I'd wake up his mother and say, Lizabeth, get up and go tend to that child, and . . ." on it goes. On situation comedy programs with a Stage 2 comic character, the straight man is typically at Stage 3, and this is the case with Sanford and Son. Lamont, who is Fred Sanford's foil, acts as the peacemaker of the family, preserving family ties and attributing good intentions to Fred's most selfish acts. In the case of the first episode related above, Lamont decides to spar with the prizefighter after Grady (a neighbor) lies and tells him that Sanford took the money hOping to buy Lamont a new truck with the fighter's winnings. In the other episode, Lamont tries to prevent his father from finding out about the psychiatrist's verdict, but when he finds out anyway, he reassures Fred that he loves him--that it's just some Of the things he does that he doesn't like. A variation Of Stage 3 is Stage 3-A Prime, or the "authoritarian orientation. At this stage, the 148 primary orientation is still toward loyalty and getting along in the in-group, but there is an added vague conception Of society as a system which is tenuously bound together and which is always susceptible to collapse in the pattern suggested by the "domino theory" of political take-over. That is, there is a belief that individuals and actions are links in great chains and that weakness in any link threatens the whole chain. The Stage 3-A Prime person, therefore, tends to view himself as an individual within a rigid chain of command, expecting automatic Obedience from those below and automatically obeying the orders of superiors. In addition, people at this stage tend to believe that breaking one law leads to the breaking of other laws, and, if unchecked, that this will lead to the breakdown of all order, resulting in chaos. In the current television season, the most consistent characterization Of the 3-A Prime orientation is Lieutenant Theo Kojak, Chief of New York City Detec- tives on the series nggk, On each episode Kojak makes some reference to the police department as in-group. He frequently tells the men that their primary obli- gation is to the department and to the men with whom they serve. In addition, he does not hesitate to publicly insult individual Officers for incompetent work performance, reminding them that the good name Of 149 the department is at stake. Kojak also bangs out orders without a backward glance, expecting automatic and unquestioning compliance. The men who work under him, however, apparently share his orientation to the sanctity of chain of command, because they accept orders and insults alike with barely a flinch. Kojak's view of law and order in the society is also consistent with Stage 3—A Prime reasoning. The good of society as such is never mentioned by Kojak, but he does make occasional reference to the spiraling nature of crime. On one episode, the crime at issue is syndicate protection Of bars. Trying to get a bar owner to reveal the name of the big protector, Kojak tells the man that, sure, it's only a payoff now, but before long he'll have a syndicate bartender, then a syndicate maid, then the syndicate will move in on his bookkeeping and before he knows what's happened, all he'll have left will be the certificate Of ownership. This may, in fact, be the mode of syndicate Operations, but it also represents Kojak's overall philOSOphy of crime and chaos. The Stage 4A, "law and order" orientation is portrayed by most of the other principal characters on drama adventure programs. Since the leader in this category, Hawaii Five-O, will be discussed in some detail in the next chapter, it will suffice to say ha? amp-j 150 here that such characters as Steve McGarrett (Hawaii Five-O), the police staff on the Rockies, Mike Stone Of Streets of San Francisco and Pepper Anderson Of Police Woman, display in addition to Kojak's orientation toward maintenance of law and order, an orientation to duty and personal responsibility that goes beyond Kojak's barrage of orders to underlings, who then take all the risks. Even in terms of police work as a job, these men and women portray a competency and dedication which leads to their being branded in the ads as "super-cops," a title implying that duty-orientation of this extreme is unrealistic in real-world terms. Although no current season prime time character clearly portrays Kohlberg's Stage 4 1/2 Of values deve1- 0pment, the reasoning associated with this stage under- lies much of the comedy on M*A*S*H. This series features the activities at a medical outpost during the Korean War. As is perhaps actually the case during wartime, traditional moral ethics are suspended by most Of the program's principal characters, although the Stage 4 or "duty" and "law and order" standard remains in the background as an ideal. In particular, the sexual ethics associated with conventional morality, or Stages 3 and 4 of values development, are put aside as situa- tionally undesirable within the wartime army. For 151 example, one Of the surgeons is married and fully conscious Of his duty to his family. This prevents him from volunteering for risky assignments but it doesn't prevent him from having a long-term affair with a sexy nurse called "Hot Lips." Seemingly, this is a no—strings relationship and no one will get hurt. Homosexuality also flourishes Openly in the camp. NO one takes any Offense at this, but when the tOp brass comes for a visit, the queen, who won't remove his dress and earrings, is shuffled out of sight. Clearly the role expectations associated with conventional morality remain an acknowledged standard by the characters on M*A*S*H, although the validity of those views is implicitly questioned by the good will which thrives in the camp, in the absence Of a rigid sexual code. Examples of Stage 5 or principled thinking have been portrayed by Doctor Joe Gannon of Medical Center. In one episode, for instance, a man convicted of child molesting had been imprisoned for a number of years while undergoing psychiatric therapy. As the episode began, the man had just been labeled as "cured" and recommended for parole by his therapist. Prison officials were reluctant to release him, however, without some "guarantee" that the crime would never be repeated. TO provide this guarantee, the prison 152 psychiatrist recommended that the molester take a libido-inhibiting drug as a condition Of parole. Before making a recommendation to the parole board regarding the use of the drug, Gannon considered all the c1aims--including those of the child molester, his wife, the raped girl (and other potential victims) and the girl's family. Despite compelling empathy (Stage 3) with the raped girl's family (her brother has become autistic as a result of his failure to aid his sister), and great personal distaste for the rapist, Gannon formulated his recommendation on the basis Of universal principles. He decided that no one should be forced by the state to take a drug as a condition Of parole. In making this recommendation, Gannon considered the effect a decision to require the drug would have as a precedent for future cases. He emphasized that the parole board must avoid setting a precedent that could easily be administered by others in the future in such a way as to deny persons their constitutional rights. Section II of the present chapter has attempted to explain the difference between content and structure as these terms are used in connection with the Kohlberg theory by l) presenting examples of actual subject reasoning at each of the Kohlberg stages, and 2) by presenting an example Of a television character at each 153 stage of values development and showing how the struc- tural level portrayed by that character was determined. Chapter 6 will carry this explanation of technique one step further by providing a detailed anaylsis of the values of the principal characters on the most popular program in each of the three prime time categories studied--situation comedy, drama/adventure and drama. . ... 154 Notes to Chapter 5 1See "Writers' Symposium" in A. William Bluem and Roger Manwell, eds., Television: The Creative Experience; A Survey of Anglo-American Programs (New York, 1967). This fact is common knowledge to TV watchers. 2Richard A. Blake, "TV's Tyranny of the Twelve Hundred," America, 23 December 1972, 551. The summary ratings, however, represent only the top Of the metaphor- ical iceberg. The Nielsen sample Of 1,200 American fami+ lies, one third of which are changed each year, is com- piled On the basis of U.S. Census Bureau data, and is considered to be 98% representative of the demographic characteristics Of the viewing public as a whole. Busi- nesses and ad agencies can gain detailed computer read- outs on the age, sex, occupation, income and predictable buying patterns of any segment Of the viewing population. This information is then used by subscribers to make decisions regarding which programs individual sponsors should support. 3Standard Scoring Manual (Cambridge, Laboratory for Human Development, 1973). 4Ibid, Story I, 2. 51bid., 6. 6Ibid., 7 7Ibid., 13. 8Ibid., 20 91bid., 23. 10Ibid., 21. 11Ibid., 36. 12Ibid., 39. 13Ibid., 42. 14Ibid., 43. 15 Kohlberg and Elliot Turiel, "Continuities in Childhood and Adult Moral Development Revisited," in Kohlberg, Collected Papers on Moral Development and Moral Education (Cambridge, Laboratory for Human Development, 1973), 22-31. 16 Ibid., 24. CHAPTER 6 A STRUCTURAL VALUES ANALYSIS OF PRIME TIME TELEVISION SERIES: FOCUS ON ALL IN THE FAMILY, THE WALTONS, AND HAWAII FIVE-O The previous chapter gave some examples Of how Kohlberg determines the values Of subjects in laboratory situations and then demonstrated how this process was used to determine the values Of some specific television characters. But, as the present chapter will show, dramatic forms themselves, as they appear on television and elsewhere, can be defined in terms of structural values conflict. From the earliest times, critics have attempted to define dramatic forms in terms Of the morality por- trayed by the characters. In the Poetics, Aristotle defined the types of drama in terms of the types Of moral character Of the characters represented. According to Aristotle, the difference between Tragedy and Comedy is “that the one makes its personage worse, and the other better than the men of present day." (Poetics 1448) Elder Olson, whose views represent those of several contemporary critics, extends Aristotle's Observations about characterization to encompass the dramatic form itself. He writes: Every emotional experience must either confirm or alter in some way our system of values; and in altering it, make it better or 155 156 worse whenever it effects a moral value. This must hold true, also, Of drama and the other arts, so that the effect of drama is its effect upon moral values . . . . One kind of drama assumes the system of values of the person of ordinary morality. It proposes simply the arousing Of emotions to the ultimate effect of giving pleasure; its aim is entertainment . . . The second kind Of drama goes beyond enter- tainment, and permits us perceptions which we should not otherwise have had; it goes beyond ordinary morality, and Offers us other and better systems of values; it, in some degree, alters us as human beings.1 Kohlberg explains that, as Opposed to Freudian critics, who see characters in drama as representing warring forces in the personality--the id, superego and ego, structural developmentalists believe that characters represent stages Of values or moral devel- opment.2 Kohlberg stresses that using the latter method Of analysis, characters represent complete personalities, not fragments of wholes, as in the Freudian view. Conflict occurs within the structural-developmentalist schema because characters representing different stages Of deve10pment have strongly Opposing perspectives on specific situations. Resolution comes either through insight (such as the "insight" that love is more impor- tant than justice) or further deve10pment.3 Kohlberg claims that the ability Of individuals to understand all levels of morality at or below their own provides a key to the appreciation of comedy and most forms of drama. To him, the structures Of 157 reasoning portrayed by characters are "filters deter- mining the writer's and the audience's perception of the characters and action of a drama." To illustrate, he writes: A low comedy in which the 'hero' amorally outwits his enemies depends upon the survival of a Stage 2 instrumental egoistic system of morality in each of us, a soap Opera depends upon the survival in us Of a Stage 3 'be nice,‘ 'be loving' moral system in which 'goodness leads to happiness.‘ He cautions, however, that "more complex literature embodies these types of moral thought in characters but lets no type Of moral thought triumph in a simple way."4 The present chapter will illustrate how the Kohlberg system can be used to structurally analyze dramatic forms on television, focusing for purposes of illustration on the tOp-rated program in each of the categories studied--situation comedy; drama; and drama/adventure. In line with Aristotle's age-Old maxim, characters in situation comedy tend to portray a mix Of conventional and preconventional behavior. Since Kohlberg's studies indicate that most Americans reach conventional morality (Stages 3 and 4 on his values development scale), the large number of comic characters who occur at Stage 2 are below the moral level Of most of the viewing public. Principal characters on programs in the drama and drama/adventure 158 categories, on the other hand, portray primarily con- ventional or Stage 3 and 4 values. These values, according to Kohlberg, represent ordinary morality, or the norm for our society. Among dramatic prime time programs, situation comedy is by far the most popular. Significantly, many Of the most popular comedies have featured a principal character whose values are Stage 2, or preconventional (that is, an individual preoccupied with his own instru- mental needs and desires). Among the tOp-rated instru- mental eogists of the past have been "Lucy" in her roles within three popular comedies of the past twenty years; Jethro Of the immensely popular Beverly Hillbillies; Sergeant Carter of Gomer Pyle; Uncle Joe Of Petticoat Junction;.Corporal Klink and Sergeant Schultz of Hogan's Heroes; and Endora of Bewitched. In recent years, Ted Baxter of the Mary Tyler Moore Show; Fred Sanford of Sanford and Son; Walter Finley of Maude; and Archie Bunker of All in the Family have topped the list Of popular Stage 2 heroes. In addition, much comedy has been created by the portrayal Of precon- ventional behavior by such children as Dennis of ‘Dennis the Meance, Opey Of The Andy Griffith Show, and Tabatha of Bewitched. The following description of the manner in which Archie Bunker's Stage 2 orien- tation fits into the structure of All in the Family 159 as a whole could therefore be applied with appropriate variations to a host of other comic characters and programs. Archie Bunker has had neither the education nor the variety of experiences sufficient to facilitate his deve10pment beyond preconventional morality. A low paid factory worker with the memory Of a depression childhood, Archie's main orientation is toward the value of a dollar. He has no understanding Of groups or of affectional ties within gorups as valuable in their own right,so he distrusts everyone whose nationality, race, religion, political affiliation, age, education, employment or length-Of-hair is different from his own. This leaves him with his wife and a few drinking bud- dies--although one of these is disqualified when Archie discovers he may be homosexual. Since Archie does not understand that other peOple's needs and viewpoints may be valid based on their own experiences, he sees his own needs, desires and viewpoints as always right, and he is only provoked to a temporary change Of mind or behavior in order to gain an immediate reward or to avoid undesirable consequences to himself. In his personal relationships, he is oriented toward direct reciprocity and exchange of favors. Archie's "best deal" is the relationship he has with his wife Edith. He pays the bills and makes 160 all the decisions and she provides him with cheerful, non-stop domestic service. An authoritarian head Of the household, Archie orders Edith around, calls her stupid and "dingbat," criticizes her cooking, her singing and her friends. But Edith remains smiling and only mildly hypertensive through all the insults, never retaliating, quick to conciliate disagreements at the first Opportunity. Archie also expects "respect" in the form of agreement and Obedience from daughter Gloria and son- in-law Mike Stivic, who are living with Archie and Edith while Mike finishes graduate school. Archie's Stage 2 values, moreover, tell him that Mike is a leech upon the household, since he receives his room and board free. NO sense Of affectional ties or pater- nal responsibility (Stages 3 and 4 respectively) cloud this vision Of his son-in-law. In one interchange which begins with Archie accusing Mike Of having no no “respect," Mike retorts with, "Archie, respect has to be earned." “So does a livin'," Archie shoots back, "but that don't mean nothin' to you." Likewise, Archie considers his work as a simple exchange of hours for paycheck; job satisfaction and commitment never appear as issues. He is careful, however, to avoid trouble that might endanger his job security. In one episode, Archie is told he has 161 to fire one of his three supervisees. These include a black man, a white man and the only Puerto Rican in the plant. TO avoid trouble with the other black and white employees, Archie fires the Puerto Rican, even though he does the best work. In another episode, Archie takes a drill and some nails home from work. NO amount of criticism from Mike and Gloria can con- vince him that the act is stealing, since the supplies came from work (about the nails, he says, "That's why the company leaves so many small things around--it prevents stealing"). But when co-worker Irene dis- covers the theft, Archie asks her to promise not to tell, saying he wouldn't tell on her if the situation were reversed. Irene leaves, disgusted, without making a commitment not to tell, however, so Archie puts the drill in his lunchbox and tells the family that he'll be going to work early the next morning to return it. The impact of Archie's bigotry on the viewing audience has been a source Of some considerable criti- cal debate ever since the series' debut in 1970. The series' creator, Norman Lear, and a substantial number Of critics, argue that Archie's impact is positive, since his comments provoke Open discussion of racist attitudes and display the bigot personality for what it really is--frightened, compulsive, going-nowhere. Others, in particular black critics, have argued that 162 Archie the lovable bigot is dangerous in that he is resurrecting racist epithets and attitudes that were at long last almost buried and teaching them to a new generation Of children.6 Still another group, which purports to take a compromise position, prOposes that Archie's bigotry wouldn't be so bad if he weren't consistently closed—minded and complacent about it-- if he owned up to his bigotry every now and again.7 But what this latter group of critics fail to understand is that closed-minded complacency as a form of moral ignorance is what creates bigotry, and it is insep- arable from it. Archie has trouble understanding the perspectives of others within his intimate circle of family and friends, so naturally he views the feelings and goals of outsiders as totally alien and potentially dangerous. Furthermore, his union, his neighborhood, even his own family, have been invaded by peOple his blue-collar, WASP upbringing taught him not to trust. All he can do is rail at the enemy. Typically, situation comedies which feature a preconventional principal character pit that charac- ter against a conventional moral thinker--generally at Stage 3 but sometimes at Stage 4--for purposes of comedy on the program. In the case of All in the Family, Archie's primary sparring partner is his son- in-law. As critics have noted, however, Mike's liber- 163 alism might well be considered conservative if it weren't constantly juxtaposed against Archie's amora- lity. In fact, according to the Kohlberg scale, Mike displays a highly conventional, or Stage 4, form Of morality. He seems to understand that the United States is comprised of a lot of different groups Of people, all with legitimate rights to reap the bene- fits Of American living. He also rejects Archie's prejudicial labeling of others, though his attempts to change Archie's outlook meet with nothing but auto- matic and sarcastic rebuttal. This is in part due tO the fact that Archie never reached the Stage 3 or "be nice" stage of morality and Mike has largely passed beyond it. As a result, neither Of them places high value on "getting along" with each other. Mike appears to value family rapport more than Archie, however. He never indulges in the type of below-the- belt insults that Archie uses against Mike and the rest of the family. A heated "it's impossible to have an intelligent discussion with you" is about as far as Mike descends. In the physical sense, Mike is no more appeal- ing than Archie, Of course. He is unkempt, lethargic, overweight and constantly pictured snacking between meals. And, too, Mike ig dependent on the Bunkers for room and board while attending graduate school. 164 In Archie's view, and that Of several critics, this stacks the cards heavily against Mike's Opinions. As far as Mike is concerned, however, Archie owes him and Gloria the extra start in life, a view shared by many real-life American families with college-age children living at home. Edith Bunker, a Stage 3 characterization, mediates between Archie's Stage 2 instrumentalism and Mike's determined Stage 4 outlook. As one critic writes, "Edith is the balance wheel on his (Archie's) erratic behavior, and the only one in the family with an Open mind. It's not much Of a mind, but it's open and kind."8 Jean Stapleton, who plays Edith, says, "I love Edith's role in the structure of this show. She's the one who states the truth. I love to be the guileless protagonist of honesty."9 Although I would agree that Edith is guile- less, her honesty ends where any type Of conflict or hurt feelings might potentially begin. Edith's entire personality, in fact, is developed around her role, not as the "protagonist of honesty," but as the family peacemaker. As Edith explains it to Gloria in one episode, in her Opinion a happy family life is every woman's goal in life. With hands folded and eyes uplifted, she goes on to describe tranquil family gatherings at Thanksgiving and Christmas, until Mike 165 interrupts and accuses her of forming her Opinions from watching The Waltons. "No real family is like that," he says. Later in the same episode, Edith tells Gloria that having babies is "the one thing that women was meant to do" ("If God didn't mean for women to have babies, why, we'd all be men ..."). She says that having Gloria made her "feel useful," and that she's been looking forward to another twenty years with her grandchildren. Edith's devotion to family life is the key to an understanding of her Stage 3 moral outlook on life. Her daily living is oriented toward maintaining close personal ties and toward keeping relationships among family members and friends running smoothly and peacefully. This is especially true in the case of her relationship with Archie. In one episode, Edith discovers that a friend is contemplating divorce because her husband is tired of her and has begun having affairs. The friend tells Edith that she and Archie are practically the only peOple she knows who are happily married. "What's your secret?" she asks. "Oh, I ain't got no secret," Edith replies with a slightly embarrassed smile. "Archie and me still has fights, but we don't let 'em go on too long. Somebody always says 'I'm sorry,‘ and Archie always says, 'That's OK, Edith.'" 166 Edith is only moved to strong words when family harmony or the loyalty Of a friend seems to be at stake. In one episode, Gloria and Mike have been fighting all day, and finally Gloria decides to sleep on the couch. Edith assembles the family in the living room and tells the story of the time her parents began an argument over a triviality. The argument went on for such a long time that deep resentments develOped, and "Things was never the same after that," Edith says. In another episode, Archie behaves so rudely to neighbor Irene that Irene leaves before eating a dinner especially prepared for her by Edith. Nonplused, Archie stomps over to the table and orders Edith to bring on the food. At first Edith refuses--asking Archie how he could be so mean to "such a good friend." She soon gives in, however, to save any further buildup Of ill feelings. Like her mother, Gloria is oriented primarily toward keeping peace in the family. Her character, however, has slowly changed and developed over the four-plus years that the series has been on the air. At the beginning, she was repeatedly portrayed in a turmoil over Archie and Mike's bickering. But more recently, she displays an independence and sense Of personal responsibility that refuses to be intimidated by either father or husband. In a recent episode, 167 Mike suddenly announces that he has no intention of having children--he'll adOpt, he says, because that would be caring for a child that is already here, but he won't be responsible for bringing another infant into the present world. Gloria is furious, not only because of Mike's decision, but primarily because he made it without consulting her. "A marriage is supposed to be a partnership, she says. Later, after ignoring Archie's tirades against Mike's virility and listening patiently to her mother's arguments on behalf of maternity, Gloria decides she wants to consider the matter further by herself. "I believe a woman should be a person first, and then maybe a mother, she says. In terms of Kohlberg's values scale, then, All in the Family's basic structure is developed around the battle between preconventional and conven- tional morality, with Archie and Mike as spokesmen for the two orientations. Edith and Gloria, struc- turally and dramatically, act primarily as mediators and conciliators. Clearly, Edith is the most sym- pathetic character in terms of morality, moreover, which suggests that creator Lear expects the majority Of the viewing audience to understand and agree with her "be nice, get along" morality (Stage 3). Another values structure commonly used in situation comedy involves characterizations that are 168 primarily conventional. In these comedies, conflict usually develops around misinterpreted intentions. Especially at Stage 3, intentions are viewed as very important, often more important than actual behavior. As a result, characters portraying Stage 3 behavior can become involved in an infinite number Of situations in which they either 1) misinterpret one another's behavior--a wife sees her husband accidentally shoved up against an attractive woman in a crowd and assumes that he is having an affair with her, or 2) attempt to avoid giving each other the wrong impression--a wife who has just taken a job doesn't want her husband to feel neglected so she begins doing all the baking, ironing and cleaning at home, although she had pre- viously had these services hired out. Though the examples cited above are about domestic misinter- pretations, mix-ups between work associates and friends work just as well. Past comedies of this ilk have included such hits as The Danny Thomas Show and The Dick Van Dyke Show (the original series); a modern example is the popular 1974-75 season comedy 32293: To summarize at this point: Preconventional, and primarily Stage 2 morality, is very important in situation comedy. Archie Bunker, as such, represents the continuation of a long trend Of popular Stage 2 comic characterizations. Archie's instrumentalism 169 is at once more racy and more realistic than that which television has previously portrayed, but his hedonistic self-centeredness is a stock and trade of comedy. Also typically, conventional morality--portrayed on All in the Family by Edith, Gloria and Mike--provides the necessary challenge to Archie's world View that pro- duces comedy. Although conventional, Stage 3 morality is sometimes the focus Of situation comedy, in which case misinterpreted motives become the primary stimu- lus for plots, conventional morality is most commonly portrayed in repartee with preconventional morality. In this repartee, the audience recognizes the inferi- ority of preconventional thinking so plainly that the Stage 2 character--who is serious about his beliefs-- appears funny, even ridiculous. In the case of drama programs on television, conventional morality is the form most commonly por- trayed. The top drama program of the 1974-75 television season is The Waltons, about a Blue Ridge Mountain family, living out the Depression years on love and the father's backyard sawmill. Although each Of the adult Waltons has some orientation to Stage 4 principles Of responsibility and community welfare, the primary orientation is toward the family-as-in—group and "getting along lovingly" in the Stage 3 sense. Conven- tional morality seems relatively stagnated at Stage 3 170 for the Waltons, moreover, since the seclusion of the rural community and the emphasis on family loyalty effectively inhibits meaningful contact with the world outside Walton's Mountain. As early reviews of The Waltons observed, the moral tone Of family life is set by the parents, John and Olivia Walton. Typically, John and Olivia's Stage 3 orientation works in favor of family harmony in an idyllic, if somewhat unrealistic fashion. Their mutual love and approval is the basis on which the entire family gains its cohesion. The parents continually emphasize kindness, caring, helpfulness and honesty as the apprOpriate means of dealing with each other and with the problems that come the family's way. Love, in the Stage 3 meaning of mutual approval and getting along with others is also the cure most Often prescribed for the troubled outsiders who, for purposes of plot, wander into the Waltons' lives. According to the message provided by the pro- gram, however, this Stage 3 love comes only within the family as in-group. Life within a happy family is the highest type of fulfillment possible according to the program. No career, no dream is worth the effort if family life must be jeOpardized in the process. As such, John and Olivia and the other Waltons portray a very limited understanding of, and a distinctly 171 negative bias against, a broad range Of lifestyles. For example, so far on the program, the professions Of acting, singing, flying, teaching and writing (except if one writes about one's family, as John-Boy Walton does) have been stereotyped as incompatible with family life and as otherwise partially unrewarding and unfulfilling. In two 1974-75 season episodes which roughly parallel each other, John and Olivia's Stage 3 morali- ties are portrayed in a manner which suggests one of the more important conflicts for the individual at this stage of values deve10pment--insecurity generated by the need for continued approval and reassurance from others. One episode is concerned with Olivia's fortieth birthday, a time Of great depression for her. She feels that she is growing Old and losing her former attractiveness and usefulness. Although a 1972-73 season episode revealed that Olivia gave up a poten- tial career as a singer to be a housewife and mother, that issue is not specifically raised here. Olivia's feelings are portrayed as somewhat vague and unfocused: one thing is clear, however, and that is that she needs reassurance from outside herself. Significantly, this comes from two younger men and in both cases takes on a romantic, schoolgirl aura. First, John-Boy reads her a romantic poem for her birthday. She doesn't 172 understand the poem, but is comforted by it anyway. Later, an airmail pilot, who has told her that he finds her attractive, takes her for a plane ride. The greatest compliment Of all, however, comes to Olivia when the airmail pilot decides as a result Of meeting her and seeing John's devotion to her and to his family, to give his own marriage another try. At the end Of this episode, life is going on as usual for Olivia Walton, now that she has been reassured that others (significantly, younger men) approve of her. Her feelings of discontent are never critically examined, however, and she never really considers taking on a new direction. Unlike many real-life women of forty who, finding that their school age children make fewer demands on them, decide to complete their educations, find a job, or otherwise pursue interests outside the home, this is no option for Olivia. It would be a ripple in the smoothly running stream of family life, and when John does make such a decision in a later episode, it almost tears the family apart. John, too, as portrayed in the parallel epi- sode, needs reassurance about his attractiveness and virility. John's problem begins when two attractive girls ignore him in town one day. That night, John tells Olivia that he has decided to take a job which will only allow him to be home on week-ends, a decision 173 that he says has "something to do with my manhood." Even before John is out the door, however, a discontent- ment begins to swell among family members. Olivia accuses him Of acting on his own, without consulting her or considering her. John tells her that he loves her and says that he's not going to let anything come between them and what they've got. The grandparents and children question him about taking the job, too, and he is unable to explain to anyone's satisfaction why the move into town is necessary. It will clearly benefit nobody but himself, since no one believes the little bit of extra money he will make is worth the father's absence. Once in town, John takes up residence in the boardinghouse of an attractive fortyish widow, Mrs. Champion. When they meet, she tells John that he seems too young to have seven children. John beams at the compliment. In a remarkably short time period, they become spouse surrogates for each other. John sits and talks with her while she makes breakfast and packs his lunch; he helps her with chores; and in the evenings, they talk about their personal lives and about his family. They have formed a sort Of Stage 3 mutual admiration society, but at the expense of Olivia and the rest of the Walton family and Of the widow's long- time boyfriend, who also lives in the boardinghouse. 174 Back on Walton's Mountain, Olivia is lonely and depressed; one night she goes to bed crying. The younger children need help with their homework, but John-Boy is too busy with his own school work. If only their father were home, they say. During the middle of the week, Olivia becomes so depressed that she calls John at the boardinghouse. He tells her that he won't be able to come home that week-end because he is being forced to work overtime or lose his job. The next day, she loses control and screams at John- Boy when he expresses frustration that his father will not be home on the week-end to discuss his school problems. Finally, events force John into a perception of the hedonistic nature of his activities. John-Boy drives into town to visit his father on the week-end and is warmly greeted by the attractive Mrs. Champion. As they talk, John-Boy tells his father that he is greatly missed at home. Later that afternoon, after showing John-Boy where he works, John takes his son to a local tavern for a drink. The boyfriend is there drinking with friends and he tells John that if he were really fond Of his wife and children, he would be with them. He goes on to say that John has upset Mrs. Champion with his talk Of happy family life and that this has disrupted their relationship. A close-up 175 of John's face reveals that he is taking the boyfriend's comments seriously. The boyfriend and his buddies start a fight, but John and John-Boy manage to hold their own and escape out into the street. After congratulating each other on how well they handled themselves in the brawl, John says he wants tO go home. Although John may have needed an exposure to life outside Walton's Mountain, his real reasons for going, which were rooted in Stage 2 instrumental need satisfaction, became apparent to him when they violated his conscious Stage 3 values. John knew in advance that his new job would be a snap. He went to town seeking reassurance, adventure, approval. In order to Obtain this, however, John immediately estab- lished a relationship with another woman much like his wife--less demanding on him because Of the lack Of children and real home responsibilities, but more demanding on the woman since it caused her to reorient herself emotionally without any hope Of a change in her previously satisfactory lifestyle. Metaphorically, that the move was an unsatisfactory abberation is revealed by its abrupt termination. John realizes his error and returns to Walton's Mountain to pursue this normal life. Like Olivia, he asks no deeper questions about the nature of his discontent and merely chalks the entire episode up to adventure. 176 Like their son and his wife, Grandpa and Grandma Walton are primarily oriented toward responsibility within the in-group and maintaining harmonious family relationships. In addition, since they are familiarly known as "Grandpa" and "Grandma" to visitors and town residents, they symbolize that all Of Walton's Mountain is one big family as in-group. An episode which focused on the grandparents' Stage 3 values centered around their decision to leave the Walton house because they felt insufficiently appreciated. Neither the family on the Mountain nor the grandparents in town are happy with the new arrangement, but both John and his father have too much pride to admit it and ask for a reconciliation. Finally, John-Boy acts as the catalyst that reunites the family. Occasionally, Grandpa violates the Stage 3 "be nice" ethic for reasons Of personal whim (Stage 2), but before the episode's conclusion, he has grown tired of his wife's disapproval and unhappiness and has mended his ways. For example, in one episode, Grandpa wins a statue at a community fair lottery which looks just like his old girlfriend. He places the statue on the front lawn in convenient view from his porch rocker. He gazes at it for several days, thus provoking a series of outbursts from Grandma, but at last, he grows tired of the harassment and dumps the statue in the river. 177 Of all the Walton children, John-Boy, the oldest son, is most focal to the program. Patterned after the series' creator, Earl Hamner, Jr., the "adult" John-Boy is now a successful writer living in Califor- nia. As such, he is relating events that happened to his family when he was a teenager, still aspiring to be a professional writer. It is his voice that Opens each episode with a brief statement of what the current story will be about. At the conclusion of the program, John-Boy-as-adult again comments on what his family learned--usually about family togetherness-- from the events portrayed. In each episode, John- Boy is on hand to watch events transpire, and frequently he has revealing conversations, with family members and others, which help the audience understand what these characters are thinking. When he is on camera, moreover, John-Boy is generally the focus of attention, with frequent shots Of his eye movements and facial expressions to indicate how he is reacting to events. John-Boy is his family's principal link with the outside world. He is attending a college in a nearby town, and he knows that if he is to become a successful writer, he must eventually leave Walton's Mountain. As an episode in the program's first season revealed, John-Boy desparately wants to maintain his close family ties while at the same time pursuing his 178 goal of becoming a writer. In part, he has decided he will do this by writing about his family. Significantly, the series began with John-Boy beginning to question what he wants to do with his life. In the 1974-75 season, John-Boy is well into his college studies and has integrated their demands into his home routine. Nonetheless, home life still comes first for John-Boy, and in episode after episode, he acts as the wise conciliator of disputes between his parents and siblings. As such, John-Boy portrays a highly equilibrated form Of Stage 3 values and shows signs of being in transition tO Stage 4. His Stage 4 leanings are revealed in his sense of responsibility, both for family welfare and toward his school work and the creative writing he continually seeks to publish. As various episodes suggest, however, John- Boy does not yet feel a sense of social or artistic responsibility to write with perfect honesty. Instead, he often sees his desire for professional prominence as a hedonistic rejection of his Stage 3 concerns regarding in-group solidarity and harmony. For example, in one episode John-Boy discovers that he has the Opportunity to publish a story he has written about the fantasized love affair of a local Spinster. The trouble is that the fragile Old woman has come to 179 believe in the fantasy, and the well-intentioned owner of the general store has already told her that John- boy's new story is tO be about her. Olivia cautions John-Boy against hurting Miss Emily's feelings, but at first John-Boy can only think about his need to publish. He offers to read the story to Miss Emily, however, before sending it Off for publication. Tears begin swelling in the aging woman's eyes as John-Boy begins. Unable to endure her pain, he trans- poses the story to read as though the love relation- ship had actually existed. Since John-Boy only dimly feels a responsibility to portray his view of society honestly for its own sake, apart from the reward he may gain by writing a certain type of story, he views his predicament as a battle between his Stage 2 con- cern for reward and his Stage 3 concern for others. Accordingly, he chooses the higher value and respects the feelings Of Miss Emily. During the 1974-75 season, second son Jason has been featured in several episodes. Jason, like John-Boy, plans a career that will take him away from his father's sawmill business. He wants to be a musi- cian and has already done some creditable composing. Jason's composing has thus far, however, brought him little but disapproval from his family. Like John- Boy, Jason is motivated to conciliate difficulties as 180 they arise, since he still places the highest value on family cohesiveness. The other children are fea- tured less Often, but indications are that teenagers Ben and Mary Ellen are primarily oriented toward a peer approval form of Stage 3 and that the younger children are still largely preconventional, or oriented toward Stages 1 and 2. In summary, family drama such as The Waltons depends on an underlying Stage 3 conventional morality which is shared by the principal characters. Conflict arises when one character violates this morality for selfish reasons, or when a character's well-intentioned actions lead to misunderstanding or trouble for others. The protagonists of this type Of drama portray both the Stage 3 norm and its violation, so that most Of the conflict occurs within the protagonists themselves. In addition to The Waltons' current popular imitator Little House on the Prairie, examples of this type Of drama in the past have included such favorites as Father Knows Best, The Real McCoys and Lassie. Another type of program, classified as "drama" by the television networks, features a professional person as the principal character/super-hero. In this type, the norm is portrayed by the super-hero but the violation Of the norm which produces the con- flict is portrayed by another character, who is rarely 181 a regular on the program. During the 1974-75 season and in the past, these programs have focused on the professions Of law, medicine and education. The super- lawyer, super-doctor or super-teacher is portrayed as possessing conventional morality, par excellance. He--there have been almost no "shes"--radiates a perfect blend of social and professional responsibility (or Stage 4) and empathy (or Stage 3). His clients or students, however, who are often personal friends as well, are less committed to conventional values. Most commonly, medical, legal or educational problems are traced by the super-professional to the client's personal problems. The solution then involves helping the client see the value of "getting along" with certain others (Stage 3) or Of assuming some type Of personal or social responsibility (Stage 4). In any case, the first step always involves the client's realization, guided by the professional, that he must deal honestly with himself and others. One popular medical drama, however, has taken up ethical issues that are central to that Of the profession. Medical Center's Doctor Gannon argued in two episodes for legalization and licensing Of mid- wives and against state-controlled behavior as a condi- In both, Gannon focused on the tion of prison parole. current law and the potential precedents involved, 182 asking the question "Is the law serving the purpose it was designed to fulfill--namely, to safeguard the right of people?" In these episodes, Gannon's orienta- tion is toward Stage 5. Unfortunately, his dialogue on the programs approaches legal jargon and the Stage 3, empathic concerns Of the minor characters dominate the moral tone. In each case, however, these empathic concerns reinforce the need for more humane laws. Like the drama programs which feature profes- sional heroes, drama/adventure programs also feature a super-hero who embodies the norms of our society. In drama/adventure, the super-hero is a law enforcer Of some type. In the mid-seventies, he is commonly a policeman or police detective, though Marshall Matt Dillon of long running Gunsmoke portrays the last Of a long series of western super-heroes. In some westerns Of the past, the law enforcer was a free- lancer, either a hired gun or a land owner Of such proportions that his or her word was law for the terri- tory. Free-lance crime fighters, even of the private investigator sort, however, are not common in modern the mid-seventies version of crime drama. In fact, the super-COp is Of a highly professional, even analyti- cal crime fighter, someone who radiates a college education and good breeding. This is true for uni- formed policemen and their non-uniformed affiliates and supervisors as well. 183 Hawaii Five-O, the 1974-75 season leader in the drama/adventure category, is a program about pro- fessional crime fighting designed for an audience Of professionals. Sponsored by Buick, Pontiac, Honda, Cashet perfume and chunky beef stew, even the ads for such mundane products as CONTAC exhibit the executive flair. (A stylish young woman speaks confidently into the camera: "This tablet--shot of a competitor with name clearly visible--contains only a pain killer and decongestant; this tablet--another shot of a cold product--contains a decongestant and an antihistimine, but no pain killer; this tablet--shot of CONTAC--con- tains all three. You have to take po_t_h of these other cold tablets to get the medical effectiveness Of one CONTAC tablet ...") The Opening credits are backed by shots of blue skies and sparkling ocean, laced with superimposed close-ups of exotic restaurants, hula dancers and airplanes taking Off. "Vacation in Hawaii!" is written all over the screen. Super detective Steve McGarrett and his assis- tants are first and foremost tacticians. Even at the scene of a potential shoot-out, it is common for McGarrett to be carrying a manila envelope of papers, reflecting the research he has just completed on the particular crime in progress and the criminal. He frequently refers to criminology studies and always 13111.. , 184 prefers the apprehension procedure that research has shown most effective. McGarrett's goal is alwaYS responsible; he wants to secure the greatest number of people from potential harm. In the case where a hostage or other innocent bystander is involved, however, they come first in his Considerations, even if this means delaying the apprehension Of the criminal. In his dealings with his associates, McGarrett is also cool, analytical and non-authoritarian. Much like a top-level executive, he invites Opinions and impressions from his subordinates, considers these and then acts according to his reasoned judgment. His principal assistant, Dan Williams, also displays McGarrett's executive charisma. He is never pictured as incompetent or in need Of close supervision; he is simply younger, and is Obviously being groomed to walk in McGarrett's shoes one day. Both McGarrett and Williams and the other members Of the Five-O team are always shown as efficient, intensely conscientious and unaffected by the personal slurs Of the uninformed. As such, they are all strong representatives Of Kohl- berg's Stage 4 Of values development. Like the super-doctors, -lawyers, and -teachers On drama programs, however, the super-professional policemen do not in themselves embody dramatic con- flict. The conflict arises almost solely from the 185 norm violation of amoral or pathological law-breakers. And furthermore, only rarely is the process that leads normal, conventionally moral people to crime portrayed. In one Hawaii Five-O episode this season, however, three women are portrayed as driven to a series of armed robberies because Of unresolved personal conflicts. One woman is married to a domi- neering slob of a man. She has no job skills and no money, and therefore fears to leave her husband though she is miserable with him. The youngest Of the three women is a junkie unable to support her habit. The third is widowed with an invalid son who requires an expensive Operation--it will cost at least $10,000. The junkie suggests the robberies and the other women reluctantly agree. At first, the woman with the sick son is sym- pathetically portrayed, and it is hard for the viewer to consider her actions immoral since a human life is at stake. As the plot progresses, however, robbery turns to assault and then murder when the junkie be- comes frightened and fires her gun. In a police battle that follows, the discontented wife is killed. The junkie and the mother get away, but the junkie tells the mother that if she gets caught, she'll blow the whistle on the mother as well. Now desperate, the 186 mother takes out a contract on the junkie. The two are captured in time to prevent further violence, however. In the Hawaii Five-O plot related above, and in the smattering Of other plots that attempt to show a normally moral person driven to crime, the overall message is that committing one crime inevitably leads to committing others and finally to the formation of an amoral criminal personality. Morality and legality are carefully shown as inseparable companions on crime drama programs. This effectively closes Off the possi- bility of demonstrating any postconventional or post Stage 4 reasoning and behavior. Had the woman with the invalid son, for instance, stolen the amount needed for his Operation using nonviolent means, the situation would be morally ambiguous--even though technically, of course, she committed a crime. Had she turned herself in for prosecution after the Opera- tion had been performed, the situation would have been even more ambiguous morally. But even in programs which profile a rationally motivated criminal, this rationality is portrayed as eroded by committing a crime. More typically, furthermore, criminals are pictured as affiliates of crime syndicates. As such, their immorality is depicted as fully formed at the 187 time they commit the crime for which they are appre- hended. The other criminal type commonly portrayed is the psychOpath. Individuals are depicted as men- tally disturbed because Of unrequited love affairs, war experiences, childhood traumas or are simply presented as deranged, killing out of boredom. Neither the gangland criminals nor the psychopaths, of course, are considered as morally rational. In contrast to the great bulk of actual crime, which stems from personal or social problems, crime on crime drama shows occurs largely in a vacuum. Even on a program as sophis- ticated as Hawaii Five-O, where McGarrett makes every effort to understand the criminal type as a means Of apprehending him, the social and personal causes Of the crime are barely hinted at. Some general conclusions can be drawn from the foregoing structural analysis of prime time TV pro- grams. First of all, as many critics have assumed, popular television programs do reflect the values of the majority of the viewing public. Although comedy portrays values somewhat below the mean for the public, this is done because the audience recognizes these values as inferior and therefore views the preconven- tional characters as "funny." As portrayed by other characters in the comedy, however, conventional or normative values provide the superior outlook which challenges the comic characters' views. 189 In non-comic forms of drama as well, conven- tional values are reflected and endorsed. The essence of family drama is predictable family unity and all family members are devoted to this goal. Since this type of in-group loyalty is a conventional norm, charac- ters on family drama are conventional, though they are continually tempted to engage in preconventional or egoistic behavior. Even television's super-heroes are revealed to be super-conventional. Their super- iority turns out to equal consistency above all else, since that which separates them from other dramatic characters is their inflexible adherence to the con- ventional mode. The implications of TV as a conventional moral force will be discussed in Chapter 8. But first, Chapter 7 will attempt to answer some broader ques- tzions about the portrayal Of men and women on tele- \7tision. Specifically, for the sum Of programs studied, ist will show the percentage Of characters at each stage (If values development by sex, age, occupational status EH1d.marital status. Historical trends and differences tuitween categories of programming will also be noted. 190 Notes to Chapter 6 1Elder Olson, Tragedy and the Theory of Drama 2Refer to Chapter 4 for Kohlberg stages. 3Kohlberg, "Moral Psycholoqy and the Study Of Tragedy," in Collected Papers. 4 Ibid. (Essay contains no page numbers.) 5See the following reviews: Newsweek, 15 March 1971, 68. Senior Scholastic, 25 October 1971, 7. See also Arnold Hano, "Can Archie Bunker Give Bigotry a Bad Name?" in Reader's Digest, July 1972, 29-34. (Condensed from New York Times Magazine.) 6Charles L. Sanders, "Is Archie Bunker the Real White America?" in Ebony, June 1972; see also Myron Roberts and Lincoln Haynes, "TV: Archie's Hang-Ups“ in Nation, 15 November 1971, 509. 7Robert Lewis Shayon, "Love That Hate,“ review in Saturday Review, 27 March 1971, 20. 8Joseph Morgenstern, "Can Bigotry Be Funny?" in Newsweek, 29 November 1971, 60. 9"TV: Speaking About the Unspeakable,‘ in Newsweek, 29 November 1971, 54. CHAPTER 7 THE KOHLBERG VALUES AND FREQUENCY OF APPEARANCES OF PRIME TIME TELEVISION CHARACTERS, BY SEX, MARITAL STATUS, OCCUPATION, AGE, AND PROGRAM CATEGORY, 1960-1974 According to television, people are their most interesting when they are: male single professional workers in their prime-of-life. These traits are disprOportionately dominant in television characters, and in Kohlberg's terms, characters with these, traits also exhibit the highest values. The present chapter will detail the findings regarding overall average values portrayed by television characters in relationship to the variables of sex, marital status, occupation, age and TV program category. In addi-i tion, the frequency of observation by demographic variable will be set forth and compared,in terms of percentage,to its respective frequency in the 0.8. population as a whole. In discussing Kohlberg values portrayed by TV characters, findings for the 1974 season will be given first, then compared to those of past sea- sons. The values of TV characters will also be compared with Kohlberg's prognosis regarding the distribution of moral values of the U.S. p0pulation. 191 192 A full presentation of data and of the techniques involved in analyzing the data are not essential to an understanding of the findings presented in this chapter and would impede its flow. Thus, the material has been placed in the Appendix. Included in the Appendix are raw data tables listing all of the characters covered in this study by program, program category and year. The sex, marital status, occupation,4age and Kohlberg value of each character are identified on the charts. The Appendix also includes a full explanation of the procedures used to aggregate raw data by variable and to find average Kohlberg values and observation frequencies. Sources and computation techniques used in compiling data for the U.S. population also'appear. The reader will recall from Chapters 4 and 5 that the Kohlberg value scale is hierarchical, with Stage 2 the lowest stage commonly found among adults and Stage 5 the highest. The reader will also recall that although each stage of values-development is associated with a specific set of characteristics, that individuals are Talmost never found to convey only one stage of values deve10pment at a time. As a result, Kohlberg's scoring technique is designed to record more than one value con- veyed by an individual and to weight the different values according to their dominance in the individual's thinking. The chart below briefly recaps the major 193 channfieristics of the values on the sliding scale used for this study. Kohlberg Value 2 2(3) 3(2) 3(4) 4(3) Characteristics in Order of Dominance Self-centered and naive Self-centered; empathy and approval oriented Empathy and approval oriented; self-centered and naive Empathy and approval oriented Empathy and approval oriented; oriented to social responsibility Oriented to social responsibility; empathy and approval oriented Oriented to social responsibility Oriented to social responsibility; concerned with human liberty and constitutional rights Concerned with human liberty and constitutional rights; oriented to social responsibility; empathy and approval oriented Values by Sex and Marital Status Table 8 on the following page gives a comprehen- sive overview of the average Kohlberg values for the 1974 TV population by sex, marital status and occupation. As the table indicates, males in general, at an average , of Stage 3(4) , portray slightly higher Kohlberg values than do females in general, at an average of Stage 3. Although the values of both single and married females coincide with the average for the group as a whole, 194 ao.ood vo.ooa ao.n a~.v oo.o om.«~ an.« an.« 00.6« on.«~ ou.¢n .vvn “awn Anvv .«.n m an.“ .m.v .n.v n n nuubu4u¢u0 44¢ a~.ou o~.vn ao.o oa.o oo.m« oo.o .o.o on.“ oo.v oh.o~ on.o~ n I I .N.n I I nIvIn .v.n .nvv Avvn eels: vodka-I ov.ma am.mv am.v o~.~ vo.o on.n :o.c no.0 o~.o~ oo.o 'w.nu n .~.n .N.n I x~cn I I ...n I ...n cusp: camcau ao.nn oo.oc~ «m.v aw.“ oo.m~ am.m oo.o o~.« oo.m~ oh.o~ on.vn . n Amvn Anon A~.n A~vn I nIva .vvn Anvv .vvn c080: «at uv.u~ an.v~ ao.o an.~ ao.o oh.~ oo.o ou.~ ov.~ ah.p o~.o ANVn I v I u I Anvv .nvv Auvn An.“ :0: vouuunl oo.om on.m> an.“ on.~ oo.o om.na av.~ oo.o am.am av.m, om.su .sen Ania ...n I xe.n .m.~ I .n.. .~.n is." no: «Hogan ov.oo ao.oo~ am.~ oo.m oc.o a~.wu ov.~ ou.« an.vn ca.n~ po.mu .vvn Amen Amvv I n An.~ .n.v Anvv .nvn n no: Hat uoHuAHwn ouunu acooaum Iaucoaoom annuaawx carcadsfl such oou>uom unaaou unAHOO aoconvmum use: uoz osum cyan: ououuauonu o>wusaocu Houoa can >9 uo cosoz osHm>Ix cowomm chad "wuuuocuszo >9 asuoum «abduct new cm: a oomum>< uOu .uwwozucouoa cu. accosuoum couuo>u0ubo can us~u>Ix omuuo>¢ can xom cenmom co«u«>oama chad ozu new noduomaooo an new usuaum assays: ma .xmm xn uuwuoouonu on» no mucousoamd mo mucosumum omuuo>< can uuouuouono >9 «0 songs» muonanou omuuo>< m mamas 195 singhanmles at Stage 3(4) portray significantly higher values than married males, at Stage 3(2) . The reasons for Unadifferences in values between men and women and between single and married males will become clear as the chapter progresses. Historically as well, males have portrayed higher values than females and, as Tables 9-A, 9—B and 9-C show, single male characters have portrayed values equivalent to or higher than those portrayed by married male characters. Single men in comedy programs portrayed slightly higher values than married men during the 1965 to 1971 period, when this pattern became more erratic. In drama programs, single men portrayed a consistent Stage 4 of values deve10pment in comparison to married men who portrayed Stage 3 values. In drama adventure programs as well, single males have portrayed slightly higher values than married males. In the case of drama adventure programs, however, the Stage 5 component in ‘the 1963 to 1966 time period may be affected by the sam- ple selections, since these included principal characters fronItflua three remaining western leaders of the sixties: Gunsmoke, Bonanza and The Virginian. A more complete discussion of the characteristics of these western heroes will follow in a later section of this chapter. fflhe average values of single versus married female characters in the historical sample have seemed 1196 I I I I amen anam Amen .m.« « amen « n I vaasu m n m Avvn I amen Amen m .«.n .«.m a«.n n n vdauun: ...n ...m .v.n n n .n.« .n.~ .n.~ .n.~ x~.n .«.n .«.n .n.~ camcam c0503 I I I n A~vn Anvm Anew Anew u N N N N caanu ANS" Amen Amen m I anam .m.~ Ana~ an.“ an.“ amen amen amen coauunz n m .MCN m “wen m n n .~.n .~.n .acn .~.m n mamcam so: vnma nhma nnma anma onma mama aama mama aama mama vama mama «ama muoaoo oanuauu> unaccomooca vpma noaouss mesa sumo» on» now usuoum auuauuz can xom an mpasoo nou.ouououunnu >9 no umsao> unabason oucuop‘ dm NAGQB 1197 a. m I I I I I I I I I I I I ..an anew m I n I I I I I I I I I voauuqt m m m .mam .maa amam I I I I I I I oamcam c030: .2m .2m I acm I I I I I I I I I odEu loam m I m I I I I I I I I I coauuuz a amaa a a a a I I I I I I I oamcam so: aama mama mmma amma omma mama aama mama aama aama aama mama mama sauna canaauo> unoccumocca mam usuoum amma canons» mama «ago» on» aauaumz as can xom mm sauna mow uuouoouosu >9 uo uonau> wquasox omuuo>¢ ma manta 1198 I I I I I I I I I I I I I Sana ...m aaam imam I I I m m m m I I I coauuax a I I .m.a amaa amaa a I I I I I I camcaa 60803 I I I aaam ....m I .aam .aam .aam aaam I I I aaasu .maa .maa .m.a I I I a a a a I I I aoauuuz .maa a a a a a a a amaa .aaa aaaa .maa a oamcaa _ co: aama mmma mmma aama amwmsumwmmaxwmmwn mama aama aama aama mama mama manuau¢> unaccomaaca aama nmsouns mama ammo» on» now asuaaa aouauuz he can xom an ousucw>p¢\osduo now uuouoouono >9 no nonaa> muonanox omuuo>< Um mqmflfi 199 more dependent on program category than on marital status per se. More will be said later about the role structure imposed on men and women by the various program categories, but suffice it here to say that comedy emphasizes married women, or more recently single women with spouse surrogates, and drama/adventure programs feature a predominance of single characters of both sexes. It is to be expected that the traits emphasized by a particular proqram category will be associated with relatively higher values. Married women in comedy por- trayed slightly higher values during the 1966 to 1971 time period, after which single women portrayed higher values. In drama/adventure programs, single females have por- trayed slightly higher values than married females, though the percentage of all women in this program category has remained very small over the years. In drama programs, the values of married and single females have remained about the same. Values by Occupation Occupation breakdowns used in this study were adapted from United States Census classifications, and are summz ri zed below . * *Classification system-- from 1970 Census of Population, Classified Index of Industries and Occupations, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C., 1971. Complete classification system includes 441 specific occupation categories (each with detailed listings of 200 Occupation Division Occupation Groups 1. Vflute Collar 1. Professional, technical and kindred workers 2. Managers and administrators, except farm 3. Sales workers 4. Clerical and kindred workers 2. Blue Collar 5. Craftsmen and kindred workers 6. Operatives, except transport 7. Transport equipment operators 8. Laborers, except farm 3. Farm Workers 9. Farmers and farm managers 10. Farm laborers 11. Farm foremen ‘4. Service Workers 12. Service workers Referring again to Table 8, the trend emerges that employed characters exhibit higher values than unemployed characters (including housewives and children). For men in the TV population, the highest values are portrayed by farm workers, service workers (mainly law enforcement officers) and students--all at an average of jobs) , twelve major occupation groups and four basic occupation divisions (groups and divisions as shown above). 201 Stm¥24(3). The second highest values are protrayed by whancmllar workers and military personnel--both at Stage 3, followed by blue collar workers, at Stage 3(2) . Forvnmen in the TV pOpulation, the highest values are gxutrayed by farm workers at Stage 5-4-3 and blue collar workers, at Stage 4(3). Next come white collar workers and service workers, at Stage 3(4), with militarthouse- wives, students and children--at Stage 2(3)—-all far behind. Not only in the 1974 season, but consistently throughout the entire time period studied, farm and service workers have exhibited the highest values. In part this can be explained as a result of the law-and- order function provided by the principal characters in westerns of the past and in the modern crime-drama pro- grams. The land owners in the westerns were portrayed as responsible for maintaining justice within their ;province; modern day "protective service" workers (police) luave taken over this function. According to television's Exxrtrayal, moreover, these principal characters in both the western and now the crime-drama carry out their roles with a high orientation toward social responsibility, a dominant characteristic of Kohlberg's Stage 4 of values development. Looking strictly at the 1974 season programs, however, it is more difficult to explain the high values 202 portrayed by farm workers, since there is only one program about a farm family represented in the sample and the format of this program varies significantly from the format of the major westerns of the past. This program, Little House on the Prairie, features a nuclear family comprised of mother and father in their thirties, two school age daughters and one female toddler. The overall youth of the family stands in sharp contrast to western families such as those portrayed on Bonanza, The Virginian or Big Valley in which parents were portrayed as middle-aged or older and where children were adults or in their late teens. In common with earlier versions of farm living, however, the Ingalls family lives in a recently settled, western community in which law and order, or justice, is still heavily in the hands of individual community leaders. Despite their youth, Charles and Caroline Ingalls provide much of this leader- ship for their town, and in so doing, exhibit the higher values associated with the older heroes of western adventure programs. Little House is always more low keyed than the tamest installments of the western formula, but its roots are nonetheless clearly in that genre. White collar workers portray lower values than might be expected though female white collar workers, at Stage 3(4) portray overall average values that are somewhat higher than those portrayed by males, at an 203 average of Stage 3. The white collar jobs portrayed in comedy, however, are concentrated in areas demanding the least education. In the case of males in comedy, this trend is particularly evident. The Stage 2 values portrayed by such characters as salesman Walter Finley (Maude) and junk dealer Fred Sanford (Sanford and Son) dilute the higher Stage 3, 4 and 5 values portrayed by the doctors and educators in the drama programs. White collar jobs for women in comedy are also concentrated at the quasi-professional levels, however, and include such jobs as bank teller (Brenda in Rhoda), clerks (Gloria in All in the Family and' Georgette in Mary Tyler Moore Show), and real-estate salesperson (Maude in Maude). The exception is Mary Richards of The Mary Tyler Moore Show who portrays the producer of a television news program. The high values portrayed by female blue collar workers can be explained by the small sample size and the fact that both females portrayed as blue collar workers are comedy characters portraying higher values than the male blue collar workers in the program. Irene, Archie's next door neighbor and co-worker in All in the Family, portarys Stage 4 values which, when juxtaposed against Archie's Stage 2 egoistic values, create comedy on the program. Rhoda, on the comedy program by that name, portrays Stage 3 values which sometimes come into 204 conflict with her husband's more dyadic value system. Joe relates primarily to the give and take in their particular relationship, whereas Rhoda orients to a larger in-group of relationships which includesmembers of her family and some of her friends in addition to her husband. Housewives (concentrated in comedy and drama programs) portray relatively low values, and this seems to be correlated heavily to their lack of employment since the average Kohlberg values of women in each occupation division contains a Stage 4 values component. Historically, there have been unemployed women in the TV population, also portraying low values, but there are Unemployed means potentially none in the 1974 season. Such in the labor force but not currently employed. Por- women have included Lucy and Viv on the Lucy Show. trayed as widows, neither had a full-time job though both were often involved in temporary employment in the hope of making a quick fortune. Values by Age Table 10 gives a breakdown of Kohlberg values by age and marital status, for the 1974 TV population. As the table shows, the highest overall values are portrayed by persons in the 20- to44-year-old age rangeI regardless of sex or marital status. Although the 20- to 29-year-old sample is fairly small (7.9 percent of 205 m m imam aacm aacm pmaaamz cmEOS I m m avvm m amom amcm amvm aavm a omauamz so: Amvm avvm amvv amvv m Hm>o a name» name» undo» mammm nu» ma vamv veIov mmIom mNIoN amuaamz xwm msam> muwnanom mandaum> Ii msumum awuaamz paw .05HM> mnwflflfiom .23 3 mafia oa mam49 I] amma ca mawpuaaaeo aa4 206 total), the 30- to 44-year-old sample equals 50 percent of the total for all ages. Within the sub-grouping of 40- to 44-year—olds, however, single men and women, at Stages 4(3) and 3 respectively, do portray signifi- cantly higher values than do married men and women, who both average Stage 2(3) . Taking a cross section by sex and marital status, moreover, the consistently highest values are portrayed by single males in the 30 to 64, or prime-of-life age range. This sample represents 43.6 percent of the total TV population. Values by Program Category The average values protrayed in drama and drama/ adventure programs are higher than the average values portrayed in comedy, as Table 11 indicates. There are a number of factors which correlate with this finding. To begin with, as explained in Chapter 6, drama and drama/adventure prOgrams are about conventional values of an institutional sort. Historically and in the 1974 season, drama programs have been concerned with the institutionalized professions of medicine and education and with the institution of the family. Doctors and teachers, whose work is most commonly carried out in large public buildings, are considered vital to national as well as personal welfare. It has been an accepted, if unfounded belief of the society that these individuals are responsible and concerned about the welfare of those 207 v avvm amvv Avon m amu09 m m amvv m m mamamm v avvm Amvv avvm amen 0am: musucm>c¢ . \wEmuo cacao hmeou c0aumasmom .m.D mmaaommumu . xmm mcu ca madam> Emamoam acommmm vamav maomuuwu Edamoam Nb mumnanom amooz aam mom mamuomamno >9 mo mosam> mamnacox vmma “anommumo Emumoam ma paw xmm an coaumasmom maazma> map can mamuomamso >9 mo mmsam> maabanox cmmzumm cowaamaaoo a HH mamfla 208 Hwy anve.l In Kohlberg terms, these qualities trans- ]ate:hmo the Stage 4 and 3 values which are in fact observed in the idealized portraits on television of such docUns and teachers as Marcus Welby, Joe Gannon, Lucas Tanner and others. Drama prOgrams which deal with the family are almacxmcerned with conventional morality since the nuclear family is an institution this country is at great pains to preserve. Accordingly, idealized benefits of the family as a socializing agency are portrayed on TV. Parents in such past drama programs as Father Knows Best and in such current programs as The Waltons and Little House on the Prairie convey to their children and others the higher Stage 3 and 4 values which make for a smoothly running society. They stress getting along lovingly with others, taking responsibility for one's mistakes, and honest.labor as a means of self-fulfillment and of helping others. Particularly since the advent of All in the Family, comedy has come increasingly to depend on a however, The brand of comedy that Norman Lear clash of values. instituted with Family and which has been imitated in such other Lear productions as Sanford and Son, Maude and most recently The Jeffersons and Hot LBaltimore, has been called "adversary comedy" by at least one critic because of its focus on characters in ideological conflict. 209 As described in Chapter 6, this type of comedy features eague-conventional or Stage 2 character in conflict with onecnrmore conventional, or Stage 3 or 4, characters. In the repartee that ensues, the lower values of the naive egoist appear as "funny" in comparison to the higher values of the conventional moralist. The net result on the average values portrayed by comedy programs, however, is to lower them in comparison to average values for drama and drama/adventure programs which more uniformly portray conventional values. It is perhaps significant, moreover, that after 1970, the year in which All in the Family appeared, female comedy characters have shown improved values. (See Table 9A.) This is in part due to the structure of adversary comedy and the fact that, so far, females on these programs have tended to portray the conventional values which come into conflict with the pre-conventional values of the principal male character. In addition, the major characteriStics of the female comic superstar have changed. She is no longer a frolicking egoist of the Lucille Ball ilk, nor is she a placid housewife like those of the previous Ozzie and Harriet, Donna Reed Show era. She is a woman with a family or other close personal ties who in general works as well. Most importantly, she portrays some concern with the world beyond her immediate in—group or family. Female comedy 210 stars who fit this pattern include Gloria Stivic (All in the Family), Rhoda Morgenstern Gerrard (33993) and Mary Richards (The Mary Tyler Moore Show). Although values portrayed in drama programs remained fairly stable during the years in the historical period when such programs were among the popular leaders, within the drama/adventure category, average values for men have decreased slightly over the years whereas average values for women have increased slightly, (See Table 9B.) The lower especially in recent years. values of women in the drama/adventure category during the mid-sixties is due to the sample size, which included only one female, Becky, in the Daniel Boone series, who was cast as a housewife despite the frontier setting of the program. Becky remained in the back- ground in the series, with participation in the Revolu- tionary War activities limited to Daniel and their son Isreal. Later drama/adventure heroines, however, including Dana on Mission Impossible, and most recently Jill Dago of The Rookies, Pepper Anderson of Police Woman and Amy Prentiss of AmLPrentiss, have been themselves professional persons involved in the action of the Accordingly, their values are molded to series' plots. accommodate the constraints of the program type, which demands the portrayal of job commitment and social responsibility, both Stage 4 characteristics. 211 As previously noted, the Stage 5 component among drama/adventure males is also a result of a limited sample size including primarily the principals from the western classics Bonanza, The Virginian and Gunsmoke. The western, however, which was the dominant drama/ adventure formula during much of the sixties, has largely disappeared from prime time TV. The Stage 5 component in.the values of such western superheroes as Ben Cart- wright, Judge Garth (The Virginian), The Virginian, and Matt Dillon, moreover, seems to be passed down not to the policemen and detectives in the crime drama programs, but rather to the doctors and school teachers. In fact, both crime dramas and drama programs about doctors and teachers appeared in the late sixties to fill the gap left by the westerns. The concern with maintenance of law and order, however, wag passed down from the western heroes to the crime drama heroes. That this should have happened in the late sixties seems reasonable given the national situation at the time. As the tempestuous sixties closed, the American people needed reassurance that law and order was possible and that society was not breaking down. Civil Rights demonstrations and demonstrations against the Viet Nam war were common occurrences on college campuses and in large cities across the country. The large following of law-and-order extremist George 212 Wallace in the presidential primaries of 1968 is one indication of the seriousness of the average citizen's concern over street violence. The crime drama dealt "better," that is, more concretely, with the law and order issues posed in the 1960's. The westerns simply could not reassure the public that the "system" wasn't breaking down. Western characters moreover could show higher than conventional values (that is, Stage 5 as opposed to Stage 4) because of the artificial, past setting in which the action was played out. In such a setting, the viewer was more willing to suspend judgments con- ditioned by a conventional moral outlook (Stage 4). It is unlikely, however, that post-conventional or Stage 5 values would be tolerated in modern crime dramas, for the reason suggested in the following example. In a Bonanza episode titled "The Honor of Cochise," which was aired in 1961, Ben Cartwright and his sons actively sympathize with and aid an Indian warrior who is pursuing an unethical Army officer. In so doing, they cross the color line, the culture line and also the boundaries of legality, since Cochise is a wanted man in white terri- tory. According to the story, however, the Army officer has lured a group of Apaches, including men, women and children into a mass massacre. Although the Cartwrights stand firm on the issue of legal retribution as opposed to the violent revenge planned by Cochise, they are 213 instrumental in discrediting the actions of the corrupt officer and in turning him over to higher officials for appropriate punishment. To translate this story into a plot for modern crime drama, one needs to imagine supercop Steve McGarrett in Ben Cartwright's role, an FBI officer in the role of the corrupt Army officer and a black or anti-war militant in the place of Cochise. It is critical here to empha-' size the situational dimensions of this moral dilemma. In the Bonanza episode, Cochise is clearly portrayed as a violent man; he kills his own second-in-command when he becomes insubordinate. In this particular situation, however, Cochise is in the right. Accordingly, in the :modern version of the episode, the Cochise figure would have to be someone with a history of violent activity and not merely a disillusioned teenager. Perhaps the Inodern militant could be associated with bomb-making (or with a past armed robbery of the type performed by IPatty Hearst and the other members of the Symbionese lliberation Army. To complete the parallel, moreover, tine corrupt law officer would have to be a member of a Imational organization such as the FBI. During the Bonanza pueriod, the Army was the national force for law and cxrder since local law enforcers were rare and police tuiheard of. It would not be sufficient for the modern- day corrupt officer to be a local policeman (such plots 214 are occasionally aired) because that makes his corruption personal and local. A large, national organization needs to ' be implicated. It seems obvious that, given national attitudes toward violence, a major commercial network would never aldxmv one of its superheroes to aid a former bomb-maker iJi the pursuit of an unethical FBI officer (if it could even be believed that there is such) unless perhaps the bomb-maker could be conveniently apprehended as well. A major impact of the Bonanza episode, however, comes from the bond of trust that is formed between Ben Cartwright and Cochise. At the conclusion of the program, Cochise has come to recognize some justice in the white man's system and it is clear that he is altered by this recog- nition. In addition, Cartwright has seen Cochise's honor and has decided to trust him. He tells the Indian that he will always be welcome on his land, and the two part as friends. Although the old western could not keep pace with the viewer's need for reassurance regarding law and order, a new type and format of western may be reemerging to meet the mid-seventies nostalgia seekers:' needs. The pilot for such a program type may be the 1974 season starter Little House on the Prairie. Although this prOgram is a family drama, its roots, as noted above, are firmly in the western genre, and Michael Landon, who 215 plays Charles Ingalls on the program, is an adult version of the Little Joe Cartwright character he played on Bonanza. .The extent to which the western motif in Little House will be imitated and expanded upon is yet, of course, unknown- A Comparison of TV and U.S. Values In general, TV mirrors Kohlberg's prognosis regarding the values of the viewing public. Recall that Kohlberg has hypothesized that women have been socialized to accept Stage 3 values as the female norm, whereas men in general more closely approximate 'Stage 4, or the mean value for the population as a whole, according to Kohlberg. As Table 11 shows, within drama programs, the values of the viewing public and of TV characters, by sex, are essentially the same. Men portray a mix of Stage 3 and Stage 4 values whereas women portray Stage 3 values. The parallel between TV and viewing public values also holds for men in drama/adventure programs, although women on such programs also portray a strong Stage 4 component in their values. As discussed above, the nature of the program category forces such women into higher—than-viewer values. In comedy programs, women reflect Kohlberg's prognosis for women in the population, since they portray an average of Stage 3 values. Men in comedy programs, 216 however, at an average of Stage 3(2), portray values considerably below Kohlberg's prognoSis for males in the U.S. population. This is related to the nature of adversary comedy, discussed above, but it may have more complex ramifications as well. It is no doubt signifie- cant that women, rather than men, portray the higher values in adversary comedy. My hypothesis regarding the cause behind this finding is that comedy programs, especially of a family sort, are aimed primarily for a female audience. If this is the case, then the writers and producers of such shows may consciously portray women as morally superior to men. Deprived (until recently) of power in the world at large, women may need to feel some type of power in the home situation and to have this power reflected in their media. Since the audience vindicates the higher values of the females by approving of them, the higher values become a type of power. Other genres which have pictured women employing goodness in the Stage 3 sense as a tool against men have included sentimental and domestic fiction and the modern- day soap opera. Freguency by Sex and Marital Status As shown on Tables 8 and 12, in comparison to their representation in the U.S. population, males make up a proportionately large percent of the TV population. .21L7 ...-onusouoa ca .ua acauonsooo can no. and. so. sauna. aouausl_an usdoaoaoo om.a oa.an on.» oa.aa oa.a oa.m oa.a oh.v oa.ma oa.aa mao.va vma.va ana.p aao.nn oaa.n aao.v ama.n «am.m amm.su hmm.~n usu9u¢u¢uu am.a. .a.voo .o.av. av.va. .a.oa. .o.ma. «cam.aa. o oa.o oa.na I ov.o a~.o oa.a oa.a on.m I o mam.o ama.m« .su.a amm.o ann.o oam.n nm~.n moa.aa solo: 00am» .oooao aa.am. .«.aa. am.ma. ma.an. .a.avo ao.am. caaa.an. I oa.aa aa.a av.m I oa.o oa.o an.a om.o an.m .>u.c ooa.an ama.n amm.v .>~.c nmo.a baa.o maa.m mmv.a ana.a case: sauna ov.m oa.aa .m.a co.aa oo.oI am.o on.o oa.n om.m aa.a nna.v ooa.am aoa.n aaa.mn ovo.o nma.a ama.o muv.a amp.v nvm.ha solo: aa - .a.m. .a.am. an.vn. .m.am. aa.va. .a.amo cc.v.aao o aa.o oo.o I no.o oo.a oo.a oa.a oo.a o aaa.o mao.o .>¢.c mam.o mam.a mma.m amo.aa emn.aa so: voauu aoooa. am.vm. am.mm. am.ma. am.vm. am.an. .m.am. ccaa.aa. I aa.aa om.a aa.o I am.o om.o oa.o ov.~ oa.a .>~.c vmm.mm mmv.n maa.o .>n.: aav.a maa.o aaa.a mvm.v omm.n so: oaoeaa oa.v oa.aa aa.a oa.o oa.a oa.a on.a oa.a cm.aa aa.m vm~.m vma.mn aaa.n amm.o ava.m amm.~ aoa.m aam.m omo.n~ vao.om an: aa< nosuo uaaao acoasua uaauaaab manuaaa: nomoamaa saga oua>uoa uoaaoo uoaaoo omma .coauaaaaoa Iaacoaaax uoz oaaa cyan: .a .a on» ac 080: Isa-5m aouauux 05. now I autos: ouao nucoouoa aucOaaaafl ca. aomma moor nausea new scauonsouo an can usuuum aouauqz an .xom an coauoasaom .u .9 on» no scauanauunao ma udn‘h 218 Whereas there is a near equal number of males and females in the U.S. pOpulation (48.9 percent males and 51.1 percent females), males comprise 66.4 percent of the TV p0pu1ation with females comprising 33.6 percent. Historically, too, if one assumes 1) that the comparative number of programs by program category has remained relatively constant during the 1960 to 1974 time period, and 2) that the relative popularity of each category has remained the same, men have outnumbered women by as much as three to one. There is also almost a direct reversal of the percentage of single and married males in the TV versus the actual U.S. population. In the U.S. population, by far most people over 18 years of age are married. In the case of men, 74.5 percent are married with only 25.5 percent single, widowed or divorced. In the TV popu- 1ation, however, 74.7 percent of all characters 18 years and over are single, with 25.3 percent portrayed as married. Since the homes of most of television's super- heroes are never visited on the programs, it would be possible to argue that some of these men were in fact married, even though the wives do not appear on the programs. My response to such an argument would simply be that these men function as single, and since wives are never seen or mentioned, there would be no reason for the viewer, at any but a contrived level, to assume their existence. 219 Though there are more married than single females in both the TV and U.S. populations, the per- cent of women portrayed as married by TV is less than the percent of married women in the population. In the U.S. pOpulation, 68.1 percent of all females over 18 are married, and 31.9 percent are single, widowed or divorced. This compares to a TV breakdown of 56.7 percent married women and 43.3 percent single women. The finding that far more women than men are portrayed as married by TV would tend to support the hypothesis that marriage is more normative and obligatory for women in our culture than it is for men. Furthermore, as Table 13 shows, the proportion of married to single males has remained relatively con- stant over the historical review period, but there are more married women in the 1974 season than there were in the sixties. In fact, for comedy, the genre in which women are most highly concentrated, single women out- numbered married women throughout the sixties by a ratio of about 4 to 1. It is interesting, even ironic, that in the pggt Women's Liberation period of the early seventies the percentage of married women on television ‘would begin to increase. It is difficult to pinpoint the cause of this trend, however, since it might just as well have resulted from an increased consciousness <3f women and attempt to portray them more realistically 220 o.ooa o.ooae o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa . o.ooa o.ooa 4‘909 nu m.m Hm I I I I a.b a.aa be b.oa I I I voauuo: m Mom I IIHwHa In.a Ihnm a.m a.» I I I I I I caudam a.aa a.m a.aa ma mi. a.m n.va a.aa o.oa o.oa I I I zmzoz M #6 am a.aa I I I a4. a.aa boa 0.0 I I I Lomuuaz N nhnmI IIa.amI awhm bMHm Ibuhm ammo .mbw a.mh Ib.om 0.0a o.ooa o.ooa o.oca onCam m ~.aa m.om m.aa m.am n.~m m.om h.aa m.aa o.om o.om o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa .fifla n o.ooa o.ooa o.o0a o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa I I I I I I I 44909 o.ma m.va I HuHH) I I I I I I I I I mmauum: 0.0m m.aa o.om m.mm m.mm plan I I I I I I I mamcLaa a can a.am a.om m.mm m.mm 22 I I I I I I I 5203 m o.ma m.aa I Hnaa I I I I I I I I I (womaum: 0.0m amm 0.8 amm Luaa m.aa I I I I I I I aamcaa o.ma v.am o.oa m.aa m.aa m.aa I I I I I I I 2m: o.ooa o.OOa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa 44909 o.mn be.am a.am 0.0m I muwHILr H.aa o.v o.oa a.m beHI m.v Im.MIII pwauaoz mmmaI .ma. m.ma IOHDN chow mwhnI m.mn o.ov o.mm nwum o.o~ mama Ixm.mm mammwm m a.mv m.~v a.~a o.oa o.ov com ...: o.vv o.mv vav o.om «.mm «.3 2953 w m.am m.a~ a.am o.on I a.ma 9.aa o.aa o.o~ m.~H o.om v.9H Im.mn Immauamz A wawwm Im.0m Im.aa chem 0.0a w.am IID.mm o.ov o.a m.mm chem). v.om m.mm oamcam N.aa 9.9a v.9v o.om o.oa o.oa a.aa o.am o.am a.na o.om a.9v a.aa mm: vmma h. mmma mmma amma omma mama aama mama aama mama vama mama mama mxmm pad (I no sumo aucmoummo >9 :0 pmaomao< mumuomumzu soar: zuas mucosomum Summons - amucwouwm cao . vmma I mama name» on» aOu muomoumu Emumoam oco nounum amuaaoz can xmm an muwuomumsu >9 uo :OaDSQauunao ma mqm<9 221 as from deliberate antifeminist backlash on the part of the media. Frequengy by Occupation Although 41.9 percent of all people are in the United States labor force, 84.3 percent of all TV characters are portrayed as in the labor force. Among the U.S. population, 27.1 percent of all people are not in the labor force for all reasons, whereas only 12.7 percent of the TV population is not in the labor force. (The remaining percentage of individuals in both U.S. and TV populations are children under 16 years of age.) Occupations which show the greatest diversity between the percent of actual and TV people in their ranks include the white collar, service and military job categories. In every case, television exaggerates the percentage of individuals employed in these types of jobs: 28.6 percent of the TV population is engaged in white collar work as compared to 18.6 percent of the actual population*; 27.8 percent of the TV pOpulation is engaged in "service" work as compared to only 4.7 per- cent of the U.S. population as a whole; and 12.5 percent of the TV pOpulation is in the military as compared to a mere 1.5 percent in the actual population. *Note that this is the percent of the entire U.S. popu- lation engaged in white collar work, not the percent of the labor force . 222 In contrast, TV minimizes the number of house- wives in its population, at least as compared to the actual U.S. pOpulation. TV portrays 8.6 percent of all characters as housewives whereas their percentage in the actual population is 16.1 percent. More single than married women are portrayed as employed on TV, although in the actual U.S. population more married than single women are in the labor force. The most interesting of these disparities is in the occupation division classified as "service." Of the 27.8 percent of all TV characters who have jobs classified by the census as "service," almost all are in "protective service," or police work. In addition, most of these protective service workers are men. By compari- son, of the small 4.7 percent of the U.S. pOpulation who have jobs classified as "service," more than half are women employed in domestic service and only about 2 per- cent are police. In other words, television portrays a police state, justified however on grounds of the benevolence and responsibility of the individual police officers. Also of interest is the finding that different types of jobs are featured within different program categories. Comedy tends to portray persons as employed within a variety of white collar jobs. Historically, there have also been more unemployed peOple in comedy 223 than in either of the other two major categories. Until the entry of family dramas in the 1970's, drama has typically portrayed white collar professionals, pri- marily doctors, lawyers and teachers. Protective service workers, and formerly farm workers, have been concen- trated in the drama/adventure category. Most drama programs and all drama/adventure programs focus on the jobs of the principal characters. This is the case because programs in these categories are abggt_work, in the sense of jobs which benefit society as a whole. In contrast, work is deemphasized in comedy where charac- ters such as Maude and Walter Finley (Maggg), or Emily Hartley (The Bob Newhart Show) may have jobs without ever appearing at work on the programs. Frequency by Age The TV population is dominated by prime-of-life adults, as shown in Tables 14A and 14B. Although only 28 percent of the actual population falls between the ages of 30 and 54, 78.3 percent of the TV p0pulation is within this age range. Furthermore, a disproportionate 50.7 percent of the TV population is both prime-of-life (30 to 54 years) and single. The median age for charac- ters in the 1974 sample was 40 as compared with a median age for the U.S. population (in 1973) of 28.4. As would be expected, given the preponderance of prime-of— 224 OHMMB a... \ 00.00a and o0.a anéu 0 a.na om.vn 00.9 wa.a 00.» «.0a 00.0 aa.a oa.9 oa.~ aN.v 00.a o0.0 00.0 OOauudx v.aa o0.0 «0.0 am.m aa.a a0.9 ua.a 00.0 on.a Canaan oa.nn aa.0 «0a oa.m on; out—a oa.n oa.0 aria 28.91 v.aa pa.0 o0.0 aa.m om.~ om.v ao.a 00.0 00.0 ooauuu: 0.0m oa.a om.v oa.aa om.a oa.ma on.“ wa.0 oa.a causam avaa ov.~ an; omaa o0.0a «9.3 on; oa.0 mafia an; aqu9 +ma vaIam vaIav vaov mnIom m~I0n maIaa aav usuoua acuau . and xom macaw 00¢ innocuous asunum auuaauz was new 60¢ an $93 ucOaunasnom >9 (ca Ham<9 225 .a.a on» an uo-uu n4 a-oauuau-ua noun aoaaaauo «use .oousoa. I cum - aammm \II, III Ia I/ .aaoa. .a.m. .a.m. .m.aa. .a.aa .o.aa. .a.aa. .m.m. .a.am. .aum 862 2.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.8 aaa.ma amm.a ama.a amm.m mam.m aoo.aa ama.oa aao.a .uo: ceases: .o.am. .a.m. .a.a. .a.a. ...o. .a.a. .m.m. .a.m. ...ma. aaa.am moa.m maa.m aam.m mam.o mma.a am... ama.a amm.am oaacaa 8.3. 8.2 8.: 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.3. ama.aoa aaa.aa mma.m amm.aa ama.a amm.aa aaa.aa aaa.m amm.aa muses. 852 8.2 8.2 8.3 8.2 8.: 2.2 8.2 mam.aa mea.a mam.m maa.m ama.m amm.m amm.a aam.o .mo: coauuu: .m.mm. .a.a. .a.o. .a.o. .m.a. .m.o. am.m. aa.m. .a.aa. mma.mm amm.m mmm.a mmm.a aom.o aaa.a amm.m aam.m aam.mm oaoaaa 861 2.3 8.3 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 8.2 862 aaa.ooa mma.a maa.a ama.aa ama.a oaa.aa amm.aa moa.m aom.mm an: aouoe +aa aaIma maIaa aanv mmIam mmIom maIaa aav usuuua . aauauu: II one son nacho 09¢ auouoaucouoo ca aucoouom «escaaaae ca muonsdzo c nounum auuausz can .xom .om¢ an .vmma «:Oaunasaom .m .o mva mqmda 226 life characters, very few children or old people are portrayed at all. Although almost 20 percent of the U.S. pOpulation is over 55, only 9.2 percent of the TV pOpulation is over 55 years of age. Even more signifi- cantly, only 4.6 percent of the TV pOpulation is under 20 years of age, although in the actual U.S. population, young peOple under 20 comprise a hearty 38.7 percent of the total. The percent of children portrayed in the sixties was somewhat greater than for the early seventies, however, and the trend against portrayal of children may again be reversing since a number of the new 1974-75 season programs feature children. Frequency by Prggram Category In the 1974 season, males outnumbered females in every program category (Table 13) although the distribu- tion was most equitable in comedy, where males represented 56.2 percent of the total characters and females 43.8 percent. In drama, men outnumbered women by about 2 to l; in drama/adventure, men outnumbered women by 8 to l. Historically, males have also been consistently over- represented in all program categories, and the disparity has been greatest in drama/adventure programs. Furthermore, women have consistently been con- centrated in comedy. No doubt this is the case because the comedy setting has traditionally been a domestic 227 setting as well. Accordingly, women can be portrayed as mirroring society's normative view of them as wives and mothers. Even on 1974 season comedy The Mary Tyler Moore Show and past comedies such as The Doris Day Show and Here's Lucy, in which the heroine is single and employed, there are always spouse surrogates available to her. In the case of the examples mentioned, these include the staff of the local news program on The Mary Tyler Moore Show, Doris's boss on The Doris Day Show and "Uncle Harry“ on Here's Lugy. Likewise, women do not fit in with drama/adventure's characterization of peOple involved in exciting, adventurous jobs. Women on TV are rarely portrayed as committed to jobs; more commonly, they are committed to other peOple. Summaryaand Conclusions To briefly recount the highlights of this chapter: - A startlingly disporportionate 50 percent of all TV characters are single males. The Kohlberg values portrayed by these characters are among the highest on television. - About twice as many people in the TV popula- tion are in the labor force in comparison to the actual U.S. population. People in the TV labor force also portray higher values than non-working persons. - There are over ten times as many police officers portrayed on television, in propor- tion to the overall TV population, as there are in the actual population. These "pro- tective service" workers portray conventional or Stage 4 values. 228 - TV is dominated by prime-of-life adults. Prime-of-life people, and in particular prime-of-life single males, also portray the highest values shown on television. Having set forth the major findings regarding the values of television characters, it now remains to evaluate the findings in part in terms of television's predicted impact on society's values. This will be the task of the final chapter. 229 Notes to Chapter 7 Recently, Watergate, the proliferation of well publicized medical malpractice suits and teacher unionization (followed by demands for accountability) have thrown the ethics of these major professions into question among the general public. 2Melvin Maddocks, "What's Become of America's Great Comedians?" The State Journal, Lansing, Michigan) 17 January 1975. CHAPTER 8 SUMMARY, SPECULATIONS, CONCLUSIONS, AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FURTHER RESEARCH Chapter One of this study demonstrated that the values embodied in pOpular culture have long been a concern of critics. Some elitist critics have tried to discredit all popular culture as inferior according to standards for elite art. Other critics such as Leo Lowenthal, Ian Watt, Herbert Gans, and Marshall McLuhan, among others, have aimed to examine the values contained i3 popular media in order to better understand individual popular works, popular art forms and the cultures which produced these works and art forms. The present study also had as its goal to determine the values contained ia television, in order to better understand both how TV functions as an art form and what TV indicates about the values of our culture. Specifically, the purpose of this dissertation has been to determine, using Lawrence Kohlberg's scale of values development, the values portrayed by prime time television characters. Just as critics have disagreed about the "value" of popular culture, depending on the standards and methods they have used, philosophers have disagreed about the locus of all values, and thus about a defi- nition of value itself. As some elitist critics have 230 231 imposed arbitrary standards for pOpular culture to "match up to," so some philOSOphers have argued that all values are intrinsic. These "nomothetic" theorists believe that values derive from the environment and come to the individual as always normative and oblig- atory. Conversely, so-called "idiographic" philosophers claim that values are located internally; therefore values are perceived somewhat differently by each unique individual. Since these philosophers believe that values should not be judged by externally imposed standards, they resemble those popular culture apolo- gists who deny the validity of aay_criticism which seeks to do more than describe a given work or art form. As Chapter Two explained, neither nomothetic or idiographic value theories seemed promising tools for the present study, given its goals. On the one hand, the nomothetic theories purport to be founded on natural laws, but nonetheless have little or no scientific evidence to back them up. Idiographic theories are even less useful in studying values, since they precisely avoid listing, comparing, or ranking values in terms of their general importance to people or to society. Since much recent research on television has been conducted by behavioral scientists, their methods and conclusions were discussed in Chapter Three. 232 Extensive studies on the effect of televised violence were conducted by the United States Surgeon General's office in the early 1970's. The Committee which coordinated the project concluded that television QQEE affect the behavior of certain individuals, and may affect their values as well. This finding was considered important for the present study, since it furnishes further evidence of television's impact on our society as a socializing institution. The methods of the behavioral scientists, pa; aa, were considered unsuitable for the present study, however, primarily because of their emphasis on behavior as opposed to the underlying values. Lawrence Kohlberg's Theory of Values Develop- ment, summarized in Chapter Four, provided the requisite practical methodology needed to study the values of television characters. Based on Jean Piaget's structural-developmental theories of human intelligence, Kohlberg's approach mediates between the mutually Opposed nomothetic and idiographic theories of value (discussed in Chapter Two) and supplies an analytical model” based on cross-cultural research, which is both philosophically sound and practically useful for content analysis. Chapter Five explained how Kohlberg's laboratory method for determining the values of subjects 233 was adapted to the study of television characters. Chapters Six and Seven presented the major findings of this study. Several tasks remain for the present concluding chapter. First, it was stated in Chapter One that McLuhan's challenge to consider television's form as part of its value message would be accepted. Since the focus of the present study has been on content, the discussion of TV form has been postponed to this chapter. Second, the major findings of the study will be recapitulated and evaluated, from the author's point of view. This evaluation involves some speculations regarding the effects of TV values on the viewing public. Some potentially rewarding areas for further research will then be suggested. Finally, the chapter will conclude by summarizing the major contributions the author feels have been made by the present study. * * 'k * Evaluating television's form as part of its overall value message adds an additional perspective to the present study. As stated earlier, it is the structural aspect of Kohlberg's theory, particularly in combination with McLuhan's theory of media values, which supplies the means whereby media form, as well as content, may be analyzed and assigned a "level of values deve10pment." 234 Recall from Chapter One McLuhan's belief that very different values are associated with television, compared to values associated with previous popular culture media. The most drastic changes, in McLuhan's view, however, have come in the transition from printed media to media that operate electrically. Although McLuhan carefully avoids labeling this transition as a progressive one, it aaaga (by applying the Kohlberg structural view), in fact to be precisely that. Considering McLuhan's analysis of media values as prOgressive, involves making an assumption about the Kohlberg technique, logical in view of some of Kohlberg's statements. It is Kohlberg's prognosis that not only are an individual's values developmental, but that the values embodied in political systems have also develOped with the passage of time. In particular, Kohlberg believes that the Stage 5, or the social contract/legalistic view of morality, appeared in government systems only after about 600-400 B-C-1 If this theory of Kohlberg's is correct, it seems logical to assume that BEESE institutions of the society, including media of communications, would similarly develOp over long periods of time. In discussing the transition from print to electric media, McLuhan is essentially talking about 235 a change of values, instituted by massive technological change. In brief, McLuhan prOposes that the following values are inseparable aspects of the print and electric media forms, respectively:* Print Values Electric (TV) Values 1. Individualism 1. Intensive Social Involvement a. Job specialization a. Involvement in "role," with strong commitment to the group or society b. Competition for b. Emphasis on external Consumer Goods consensus and community (ala Horatio Alger welfare myth) c. Separateness of c. The "Global Village” peOple in time and united by phone, radio, space TV and airplane d. Competition d. International conscious— between nations ness and cooPeration ("nationalism") 2. Visual Standardization 2. Visual Nonconformity a. Uniform social a. Group-derived norms, codes, arbitrarily based on consensus adhered to b. Standardized b. Consumer goods reflecting consumer goods life styles and ethnic (ala the assembly affiliations (ala the line and depart- "boutique") ment store) By comparing Kohlberg's hierarchical scale of values to the values McLuhan associates with print and electric media, McLuhan's print values appear to be essentially pgafconventional in the Kohlberg sense, whereas the electric values appear as conventional *These values are taken from the discussion of McLuhan in Chapter one. 236 according to Kohlberg's theory. The emphasis on individualism as defined by McLuhan, associated with print media, implies concern only with that which bene- fits the self. This, of course, is a pre-conventional characteristic, in Kohlberg's terms. Associated with individualism is the naive assumption that what benefits the self is somehow good for others as well. This assumption is illustrated by McLuhan by reference to the Horatio Alger myth (a common theme in literature), which he says television has destroyed: In our electric age, the one-way expansion of the beserk individual on his way to the top now appears as a gruesome image of trampled lives and disrupted harmonies. Such is the subliminal message of the TV mosaic with its total field of simultaneous impulses.2 Again, in Kohlberg's terms, the Horatio Alger myth-—' with its naive view of human interrelationships and its emphasis on wealth and property--is clearly a Stage 2 representation. The print value of visual standardiza- tion also seems pre-conventional in Kohlberg terms, since it implies automatic adherence to arbitrary codes and pre-set standards. By contrast, the electric values of intensive social involvement, adherence to external consensus, involvement in role and commitment to society, sound very much like Kohlberg's "conventional" level of 237 morality, at which individuals are oriented to in-group love and approval, to group-derived norms, and to social responsibility. If it is true that the form of the television medium is portraying conventional values as well as the content, then McLuhan is right, and the medium ia the message. As detailed in Chapters Six and Seven, the findings of this study indicate that TV portrays primarily that conventional morality (Stage 3 and Stage 4) which Kohlberg believes is the norm for our society. Typically, these conventional values are portrayed on drama/adventure and drama programs by single, profes- sional males in their prime-of—life, although women on comedy programs tend to portray conventional values as well. It is important to emphasize, however, that the single male professionals, despite their life styles, g9 exhibit conventional morality specifically in Kohlberg terms. Even the superheroes in current crime dramas and in former westerns--whose counterparts in popular fiction are often loners--have close rela- tionships with work associates who serve as family surrogates. In-group loyalty, approval and harmony (Stage 3 characteristics) are thus stressed by the characters on these programs. In addition, these characters display an orientation to society's laws 238 and to social responsibility which transcends in-group loyalty--qualities associated with Kohlberg's Stage 4 of values development. Since a recent comparative study of television formulas lends support to the findings of this disser- tation, it is appropriate to review it briefly at this point. Horace Newcomb's TV: The Most Popular Art has as its central thesis that all television formulas-- including those he calls situation comedy, domestic comedy, westerns, mysteries, doctor and lawyer shows and adventure programs--are more like each other than like the literary formulas they are descended from. As Newcomb puts it: The crucial point is that in adapting the clearly recoqnizable forms of pOpular enter- tainment for its own uses, some of the distinctions begin to fade, and the forms are changed into specifically television versions of themselves. Those versions often bear more resemblance to each other than to their non-television counterparts. Recent television westerns are directly related to recent mys- teries and both bear strong resemblance to the formulas we have already discussed, the situation and domestic comedy. For the world of television is a world of explicit values, and in the evolution of forms we see more and more how values from one segment of that world filter into other segments, and ultimately into our own 1ives.3fi As the quotation implies, Newcomb believes that the domestic and situation comedy formula is television's 239 basic model, after which other formulas have become increasingly patterned. He cites the appearance of family surrogates in programs of all types and further notes that "intimacy," pa£ aa, has become a value of television formulas. According to Newcomb, television formulas portray values-~such as love, family cohesive- ness and concern for others--which are associated with an older, idealized era in American history. Newcomb's study also supports the notion that TV £953, as well as content, must be evaluated when analyzing those values portrayed by the medium. Like Marshall McLuhan, Newcomb believes that a major portion of television's value message comes through the form of the medium, though he concentrates on the physical and production constraints of TV as opposed to the electronic means of transmission. In particular, he believes that the sense of intimacy (with its Stage 3 overtones of "getting along" peacably within the in-group) is created in major part by the smallness of the TV screen, a part of one's own living room; and by the fact that only a few faces or forms on it can be distinguished clearly at a time. "Television is at its best," he says, "when it offers us faces, reactions, explorations of emotions registered by human beings."4 240 All this leads to the question of what effect TV has had on its viewing audience. As Chapter Three explains, behavioristic studies of television lend significant support to the hypothesis that television agaa influence the behavior, and perhaps the values of the viewing audience-~although the degree of influence remains in doubt. In addition, Kohlberg, Piaget and the other developmentalists believe that we learn in part by witnessing higher levels of reasoning and higher values in others. Clearly, television provides ample role models, and it seems reasonable to assume that the thinking and behavior they portray have an effect on the viewers. If television values affect the viewing public, it is both reassuring and alarming to realize that TV is such a consistent force for conventional morality. It is reassuring to know that children, adolescents and other pre-conventional moral thinkers have so many strong models of conventional morality to emulate. It is alarming, however, to consider the possibility that, by its sheer pervasiveness in our lives, TV works to inhibit adult deve10pment beyond the conventional norm. Despite perhaps justifiable concern about the effect of televised violence, the present study would indicate that overall, television should have a 241 pggfsocial effect on children and pre-conventional adolescents. Recall from Chapter Three that the Surgeon General's Committee reported the findings of one study, conducted on children, which supports this conclusion. The study found that TV viewing can have a pro-social effect, particularly on youngsters from lower socio- economic backgrounds, if pro-social prOgrams are viewed. Conventional morality, of course, is pro-social for this culture (and for other cultures, if Kohlberg's cross-cultural data is valid). In the case of adolescents whose values lie below the norm, TV should also accelerate the movement toward conventional values. In fact, according to McLuhan, television has ended adolescence in the United States. Certain legal and social changes that have tran- spired since UnderstandingMedia was published (1964) support McLuhan's theory. 'For example, within the past decade the voting and drinking ages have been lowered to 18; in addition, the age at which females can obtain contraceptives without parental knowledge and consent is continually being reduced. Perhaps most significantly, the Viet Nam War was in effect ended by a generation of men in their late teensand.early twenties who refused to fight a war they didn't believe in. Finally, young people are among some of the most active participants 242 in the new personal growth and group-involvement movements which have emerged since the late sixties, including Transcendental Meditation, Transactional Analysis, empathy counseling, and the charismatic reli- gious movement. Superficially, at any rate, it seems that young people are acting out conventional values at an earlier age than before, and that television may be responsible for this change. Some pre-conventional adolescent and adult citizens, however, lack practical means to act out conventional values in society. PeOple in this sub- grouping include the poor, the undereducated, and (until recently) the young and many members of minority groups. The effect of TV on these people may be to produce aagiesocial attitudes and behavior. This seems especially likely because television encourages in-group identity and social responsibility but cannot, in itself, provide access to socially committed roles. In large part, this is because "roles" are so closely linked to “jobs" in our society. The poor young black person who becomes distressed about the conditions of ghetto life as a result of watching TV, for instance, cannot necessarily improve conditions for his peers or even find a socially responsible job. Because the TV values of empathy and consensus may be encouraging understanding and inter-group harmony, television may also ultimately 243 cause barriers between groups to be broken down, however, thereby allowing society's benefits to flow more evenly to all its citizens. For the majority of Americans who are already conventional moralists, television may work to inhibit further development. To repeat from Chapter One: 95 percent of all U.S. homes have at least one television set and the average viewer spends 1200 hours per year watching TV. As continuous exercise builds up certain body muscles, so should it be expected that continuous television watching would strengthen a person's belief in values reinforced by TV, making these values ever more resistant to change. That TV may inhibit post-conventional develop- ment is alarming because of the differences between conventional and post-conventional morality, according to the Kohlberg scale. People at Stages 3 and 4 of values development are only as "moral" as the society in which they live. Hitler appealed to the values of in-group loyalty and approval, and of social reponsi- bility, to gain support during his rise to power in Nazi Germany. In Kohlberg's belief, however, the authors of the U.S. Constitution, as well as the defenders of its principles, have been Stage 5 thinkers. If our democracy is based on the quality of thinking 244 that questions norms rather than blindly accepts the prevailing mode, then it seems logical that the more Stage 5 individuals in all walks of life, the better and more open that life will be. To the extent that television increases complacent acceptance of existing norms, and in so doing precludes or delays post-con- ventional development for some peOple, it is a regressive influence in this society. The 15 percent or so of the U.S. population which is already oriented to post-conventional or Stage 5 or 6 morality is not likely to be affected by values transmitted by television, since there is nothing for them to learn or affirm from TV in a values sense. It is important to note, however, that if McLuhan's "electric values" can be translated into Kohlberg's "conventional values," then post-conventional peOple aga, contra McLuhan, able to see TV for what it is and to put it into proper perspective. What, then, is TV's future? There is every reason to believe that the values transmitted by television will not change in the foreseeable future. This supposition is supported by the historical analysis included in this dissertation. The distribu- tion of value levels within TV content has remained essentially constant over the years under study, 245 regardless of the fact that this period has been char- acterized by an accelerated amount of social change in the area of equal rights. Programs featuring minority characters are becoming common, but they follow the established formats for the various program categories. Equal rights, pa£_aa, are never at issue on these programs; in-group harmony and social responsibility are stressed as primary values. It is clear that television is capable of portraying post-conventional values in a manner appealing to viewers, since there has been a smattering of popular Stage 5 characters. Documentaries of an issue-oriented nature and news specials of various sorts also seem to be gaining increased air time and popularity in recent years. Since post-conventional morality is analytical, questioning and issue—oriented, this type of programming perhaps holds out the greatest hOpe for advancement in terms of television's overall value message. An exploration into the values portrayed by this type of program is beyond the scope of the present study, however. The findings of this dissertation suggest that much additional research in this area is required. First, there must be a much broader and deeper study of more programs, more types of programs and a greater historical coverage of programs. Fortunately, more 246 information on past programs should become available, with the increasing establishment across the country of popular culture repositories, which include television scripts and videotapes of past programs. More SOphisticated statistical sampling techniques are also needed, incorporating detailed Nielsen data on past programs. For a comprehensive study, access to the actual Nielsen reports, as opposed to summaries reprinted in trade publications, would be crucial. If possible, computerization of the computational technique would be extremely helpful, since this would allow more data to be analyzed faster and would offer the possibility of multivariate analysis. In addition, a detailed survey of all relevant literature on media values transmission is indispensable for further study. This should include all significant behavioristic studies, as well as studies utilizing independently-derived models and studies of a more subjective nature. Other projects of a related nature are also suggested by the findings of this study. Since single, professional, prime-of-life males predominate on prime time television, it would be interesting to know what percentage of scripts are written by men rather than women. It would further be interesting to compare authors and producers with the principal characters 247 in programs they have written or produced, in terms of the demographic variables selected for this study. Finally, daytime programs, especially the daytime dramas, warrant analysis and perhaps comparison with evening programs. On the one hand these programs appear to deal seriously with such issues as divorce, unwed motherhood, alcoholism and abortion, well beyond the limits of prime time television. On the other hand, the viewing audience for the daytime serials consists primarily of housewives. Not only does Kohlberg believe that socializing practices for all females have tended to reinforce Stage 3 morality as the female norm, but also housewives, in particular, act out society's most conventional female role. It might therefore be predicted that complex issues are resolved in the day- time serials by using conventional, Stage 3 logic. If this is the case, then conflict might be expected to originate from Stage 2 characters as it does on prime time comedies. It would be especially interesting to discover similarities between the evening comedies and the daytime serials since the audience for the comedies is also projected to be primarily female. It is my belief that this study has made several contributions to the field of knowledge in American Studies. First of all, the study has contributed 248 toward a better understanding of pOpular TV formulas. Determining the values of television characters uncov- ered the preponderance of conventionally moral charac- terizations. In the case of comedy, the manner in which conventional and pre-conventional characters are pitted against each other to create humor was illustrated. In the case of drama programs, determining characters' values showed that they accept conventional roles and norms and that tension and crisis are created when these norms are violated. Resolution comes in drama programs when characters reaffirm their traditional values. Finally, in the case of drama/adventure, the discovery that principal characters exhibit conventional moral values allowed for a better understanding of pOpular culture's aapaghero as embodying, rather than transcending, cultural norms. Likewise, the study suggested that the Kohlberg model would be a valuable tool in studying other works of popular and elite culture. It has traditionally been argued that the "vision" or "world view" of the elite practitioner is more complex and more complete than the popular artist's is. Within the area of human values, the Kohlberg scale could be used as one means of testing this hypothesis. In addition, one study I did of William Faulkner's Absolom, Absolom! indicated that applying the Kohlberg theory can yield a meaningful 249 new reading even to a well-critiqued classic. My study revealed the growth of post—conventional thinking in Charles Bon and thus made his sacrifice of life a more integrated part of the novel's ending. Analyzing the values of the novel's various narrators also helped filter out and explain the unreliable portions of each version of the Sutpen/Bon story. Kohlberg himself has suggested that his theory is a helpful aid in studying tragedy. The present study indicates that the potential applications are very broad indeed. The study has also contributed toward a better understanding of American cultural values since Kohlberg's hypothesis that "conventional" morality is normative for our society was supported by this study. Certainly conventional morality is normative for television, America's most pOpular art form. The study suggests that conventional morality may even represent an unattainable ideal for some peOple since drama/adventure characters portraying Stage 4 values are so often tagged as superheroes. In particular, performing well in a job which benefits society as a system may be viewed as the highest possible ideal by many Americans. In-group solidarity, especially family love and harmony, would also appear to be "high" values in the perception of most Americans. "Getting ahead" per se, in the sense 250 of materialistic gain, is 99E a value endorsed by tele— vision. Thus, per McLuhan, television is modifying the American dream of wealth and esteem to a dream of a moderate income combined with a responsible job and ample close relationships with family or work colleagues. Finally, this study has confirmed the criticism of many feminists that men and women are portrayed unequally by television. The Kohlberg analysis showed that women on TV, on the average, represent lower values than men. In addition, frequency comparisons showed that men as principal characters outnumber women by as much as eight to one in the drama/adventure category, while they predominate in every other category. A majority of these men are presented as single, dedi- cated professionals, in their prime-of-life age range, so that women are only episodes in their lives, according to TV. Women characters, on the other hand, are concentrated in domestic comedy and family drama, where they appear as married or affiliated with Spouse surrogates. Thus women are shown almost exclusively as interdependent with men for peer relationships and peer approval. Most men, however, are pictured in primary relationships with other men, usually work associates. 251 Though in general there are no sexual conno- tations to the relationships between men on television, the recurrent pattern of these relationships surely provides another example of the theme of "cultural homosexuality" which Leslie Fiedler ascribes to much of American literature.5 More importantly, however, the fact that there are numerous sets of male peers on TV and almost none of women, unless they are rela- tives, indicates that female peer relationships are not seen as normative (or even possible?) by our culture. Clearly women are expected to find peer approval and a satisfying relationship only with a man. Men, on the other hand, can meet these needs for each other. I intend to explore some of the ramifications of this finding and to trace its appearance in other popular media in a forthcoming book—length study. 252 Notes to Chapter 8 lKohlberg, "Moral Psychology and the Study of Tragedy," in Collected Papers. (Essay has no page numbers.) On the subject of moral development throughout cultural history, Kohlberg cites L.T. Hobhouse, Morals in Evolution (London, 1906). 2McLuhan, UnderstandingMedia, 257. Horace Newcomb, TV: The Most Popular Art (New York, 1974), 65. 41bid., 245-246. See Leslie Fiedler, Love and Death in the American Novel, rev. ed. (New York, 1966). APPENDIX A PRIMARY DATA TABLES The following tables present the unaggregated data collected for TV programs and TV characters for the years 1962 through 1974 which this study covers. The data are presented first by program category (i.e., comedy, drama, or drama/adventure), then by show title, followed by year. Characters fall under the classifi- cation of the show on which they appear, and the relevant demographic and values variables are shown for the character to which they apply. A list of explanatory abbreviations appears at the end of the primary data tables. The following variable values were estimated by the author: - Age of character - Occupation of character where not clearly stipulated - Marital status of character where not clearly stipulated. Judgments about the values of characters included in the analysis were made using the procedure detailed in Chapter 5. 253 254 N 0a :3 as: x unsuu¢ N om m an: a soa>a> m on ma saw 9 xosuomscz .uuz N on :3 an: : moacam nouacz “Non om . :3 as: h >0aCah spam: vhma mono: m on a: cam z umomm N am a: Cam 2 woocaax N ov as am: 2 xcmam aNom on a: Cam 9 amaa 00: v ov a: Cam 8 aocanu aN\aIvvv mm a: saw 2 umaoma9 AN\aIv2v mm a: cam z o>0x3m= vhma mamc¢cx n mm m an: m :uouacoouoz mpa m mN :3 saw a mocmum anm am am am: 2 pumuuwo moo m mm am um: .m vuouuou 2005a vhma uconx “mom am am can 2 pm m mN am Cam 2 ovanu vhma an: on» a ocasu m on :3 cam : pancsm uCOEma N om :3 cam z oancsm pmum amma now can cucucom v av am am: a wcoua v mN um am: 2 oa>aum oxa: Avon aN :3 an: m 0a>aum saanU m we r us: a umxcsm nuapm m as am .8: 2 amazon 3.8.2 amma mania as» ea aa< mayo-Bo moa<> mod .UUO 449am¢2 xmm mm9u UHmo Edduomm mammamc¢ Cowman vmma qu mumo 255 v 0a :3 can 2 aamaocaum nIvIa 0v :3 can 2 nocco9 mound vmma uossu9 sauna M 0a :3 saw a answcou amov mm :3 saw 8 hoaax conmmum .maa ca :3 cam z mnaos muons: amma .o.: .mnaoz sauna: v om :3 :am . z Hmcsuoq aaov 0v :3 can : coccmo woo chma Houcmo anoapmz MIN a no :0 h manna m ca :0 :0 h xumz nIvIa mm am an: m uaammca ocaaoamu mauanum on» ance am as am: : uaamoca awauwcu vmma co mason mauuaa a nummeaam aIN aa :0 no 2 non an m aaum “Non ma :0 no 2 com aNcm aa um no a coaam mum: n ma um no 2 cemmo Avon 0N um :am 2 couamz momIcnon anm on am as: : neuamz maocmuo m om : an: a couamz mapcmuu m ov am am: : cODaMS 2200 m ov m an: m couamz ma>aao vmma acouauz 0:9 magma N 0v I am: a maaamnm m mm :3 saw a ouuomuomo Avon me 23 um: z xmuasz m 0v :3 :am a swaam mom N 0v :3 Cam 2 nouxmm 009 aavm av :3 Cam 2 ucmuo .uz 303m a an a; :aa a aauaaoam mam: aama once: scams man: age maosoo moq<> mod .UUO A¢9am¢z xmm ma90¢m¢=u mzmmAmHz ACE 24m00mm amoum9 Damadmoozmo 2H mudmw z¢muomm .vmscaucooo ma manm9 256 .cmmonu no: :Oaaao van: use .haso aamaocaum oco so usOOu ou humanooo: cucuouonu on: an .nopOaamo u.comsou on» no ucoouom 0a causasoaum on» can» once msasoa> popsaooua uuuaaucoo ocaasoozon can nasmaocaun uo oocnuoomma on» ocauouou uo>ao>ca umEuom oxOEmcso amma on» monsoon hoops many now po3oa> uos ouo) unauuomu can soon: nuouuuuonu anoaocaumc c mm on Cam 2 amazouu v mm mm saw 9 uwmmwm vnma £0603 muaaom aaov 0m mm can 2 coaaao uumz vmma coxosmcsu amvv on am can : mambo 82 a. on mm in z 3.89 Avon on mm as: a omao aaao Amov on mm an: 2 Como oxaz v om om cam z umxmm .umw aama umaxoom 0:9 v mm mm Cam 2 umxumm v 0a mm Cam 2 noxUOuO aoaaum ¢m2m om mm cam z xmnox .ua vhma anox amoN ma 3 Cam 2 pneuxoom xxoom amoN av om can 2 cacaxoom an vmma nuaam chauxoom c mm mm cam z uoaamx m>ouw oouaocouh v mm mm cam : 0:0um mxa: vmma saw no uuoouum v mm mm cam z usuaaaaz coo a 0v am can 2 anonymous o>wum «mma 0Io>am aaosms ouauso>u¢ \oaduo mo4<> mud .UUO A¢9Hm¢2 xmm mm9u¢m¢:U_ mzmwamaz m09 z Uammdmoozmo 2H mm¢m> z xONQ N mm :3 um: S hmcooz .uz AMVN me 3 Cam m >w> N me a cam m Nona honmmma zonm Nona AmVN ov :3 Cam m hmzmzumm mmaz N cm :3 um: z mamvm>uo .uz ~IH ma 9 cfim m No2 >Ham N on D Cam 2 cucumo m on 2 Cam m >ccmuo 3; cm. :2 :8 z Bz 3.32 3:232? 3325 m ow : Cam m mam ucs< m me mm cam z chumm N n no so 2 >mao m 3 mm 5.6 2 >92 5.32 30$ 533.6 :22 2a. .6980 maq<> mo< .uoo q onmmmoozmo zH mm¢m> admuomm mammamcd Havauoumwm uOu mama wH manta 2558 n on N: can : uuoam N mm N: can 2 uamaNNz N ow N2 an: x xcaum ANVN mm N: aNm a mafia no: N ov N: :Nm : NocoNou v mm a: :«m z Hommmua v mm N2 cNm z mamxxmx NNNN =.m.¢.: N on a: cNm x ououcom ucoemq N on :3 cNm z vuoucam cwum NNuNNmN now can ououccm v me am um: m mcmnH v mN um um: z oN>Num waz Avvm mN 23 um: m oN>Num «NuoNo N my : um: m umxcsm :uNwm N me am um: z umxcgm mNnou¢ NNuaNNN NNNeum an» a“ NN¢ N cm :3 cfim : amazes m «H no no 2 >ccmo N NH :2 no N musmq m NH :3 cNm 2 cume m ov n3 :Nm m Nmflufinm NN-¢NNN NNNeum mmnNuuumN ANVN N no no 2 Nuon ANVN N no no N Nausm m NH um no u Nmmam A¢Vm om wm cam z nocmum .uz Avvm ov as cNm z NNNN oNoco cN-NomH uNoNu¢ NNNeom N mm a: um: z Nuumq N mm D um: m muoocm N N no no N «nuance m mm a: um: z :Nuumo m mm = um: m unucm2mm mon.mmN nwnoufiaom N om ,Nz cNm z Nadsgom .umm q ov N2 cNm z ammo: mmma cacao: u.:ooo: Nausea maq<> mu< .uuo q ona z<¢oomm Avoacaucoo. ea canoe 259 cm :3 :Hm m oHHosacou mm :3 ch : NoHHx o>uum oo :3 :Hm z anHm: anon»: NNnoomH .a.: .NnHuz cacao: cm :3 saw : umcnooq . av 23 ch z coccmo won NNuammH noucmu HooHvoz numnmNHHm HH no no 2 non an :wum mH no no z couHmz cmm mH am cu m couHaz :mHHm Nun: NH um no 2 acuHmz comma ON um :Hm z couHmz Nomuccon oN Hm um: z coume massage ON : an: m couHmz meucmuu ow Hm um: 2 acuHmz anon . ov : um: m couHmz mH>HHo NNNH acouHoz 0:9 mm :3 Cam 2 cmEmsmx .uz mN n2 ch m onH< mm :2 ch m NHH mm :2 ch : coxHo wuwm HNnmme NNN zoom «aqua ON a um: m mHHHmnm om Hm ch m mvogm mm :3 ch m wuumwuomo mv 53 um: z Nouns: ow :3 :Hm 2 umuxam owe me 53 um: z acmuu .uz mm a: cam m acuonowm Nun: MNINNmH 302m ouoo: uoahk and: N om :2 um: : unguu¢ N cm a um: m cmH>H> m mv mm um: m acm>m mcHuon N om n3 um: z umuHmz , AN. n on :3 no: .m mass: 212.: 09.8: Noon—cu maH<> mom .uuo HaeHm onmamoozmo 2H mama» :cmuomm Amvflfiflfiuun—Onvv 0H GHQ”? 260 9 mm mm saw : uadwaaas can v ov 0m :«m : uuouuuouz 0>mum nuuonma clo>wh «gnaw: .3 v NN om 5m m 033. AN: NN mm :3. z 2.3 Hmvv mN mm .:Nm 2 oumm .qu on em :Hm : umwuo :Hnummo oNuNoNH canvu cos c mv om :Nm 2 hndoo EOE v mv mm :«m z ocdxmum mN3wq ohlmwma .H.m.h one v NN om ch z NHHHz v mm mm cNm z Hamcuwm v ov mm :Nm m mama v ov wm Cam 2 mwumm v ov mm cam : mmHmnm moaa canaanoman coNuuNz .vvm .va :0 no 2 ammumH N NN um 52. m 560m v mm mm um: : Hwflcma mwnmwma ocoom Hoacmo Amvv ow mm saw 2 sauce macaw HNVN NN mm ch : coHchuH> use NN-NNNH cuHchuH> one .vvm mH no no 2 mNEmn HNIONmH Amvv mN mm :Nm 2 won oauqu mnvlm ov mm :«w 2 Emvd Anvv mm mm :Nm H z mmom mnvnm ow mm Cam 2 unmwu3uumu com Hulamma oncucom Amvv om mm saw : coHNNa uumz nhuouma oxoancnu q me 23 :Nm 2 Houmooz x0520 . q me :3 :Nm 2 nosoHHsouz ucNHm monomma dawns comm: eunuco>u< . \nadua uni; mod .000 4.48ng xmm mafioésmu mzmmAmHz mg 2550mm among—640 0mmmqmox mmqdemts UHmmdeOZm—d 2H mmdmw 2580mm HooscHucoo. NH «Hana 261 Auauuuu Advance mada4uuoo nasaoo ca ma Moons can own no tuna» 0a Moon: «an. vauno I an Ramada Ho own «0 undo» ea. unovsum I am Avaonounon uou oflnwncomuwu uov Oufi3munom I m NuouHHHz on» cH n H: oozedmemco I a uuxuoz Sham I am noxuo3 moN>umm I mm umxuoa umHHou wsHm I Hm umxuo: uoHHou muNn3 I :3 coauumz I um: mHmch « ch coana>wo HucoNummsooo I .000 . magnum Havana: n Hmuflum: OHGEUW I h 0H6: I 2 v mm mm cam : umxoom v ov wm :«m z uuxoouu HoeHum «NVN om mm ch z xunox .uq NNNH xmnox Amvv om mm :Nm 2 mwunu Anvv om mm cam : auums Avvm om mm um: u come adflh Amvv om mm um: 2 came wxfiz e om mm cNm z umxam .umm NNuNNNH uwaoom may v om mm :am 2 chcm2 won Nhlahma xdacmz “Nye m¢ wm cam 2 wvmu Bum HNmH Nassau n.0vuo Amvv av :3 Cam 2 0x50 Hm.e mm :3 :Hm m «xxfiz Amvv ov :3 cam 2 mxflz HNmH umouuumcoq ousuco>u¢ \oadua msqd> mom .UUO AdBHm<2 xmm mmau UHzmdmoozmo 2H mm