MSU LIBRARIES 4—7:“. RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped beIow. 4’59 2’ Saw 5: T‘fIWJ ”‘0, .- 3‘ MAR 12 15 MM THE BIRTH OF THE FIRST GRANDCHILD: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY OF THE TRANSITION TO GRANDPARENTHOOD By Cathleen Erin McGreal A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1984 Copyright by Cathleen Erin McGreal 198A ABSTRACT THE BIRTH OF THE FIRST GRANDCHILD: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY OF THE TRANSITION TO GRANDPARENTHOOD BY Cathleen Erin McGreal This study examined grandparents' expectations for the grandparenting role before and after the birth of the first grandchild. The hypotheses investigated considered whether (1) the timing of this event has an impact on perceptions of the role; (2) feelings of biological renewal are stronger for maternal grandmothers and paternal grandfathers than for paternal grandmothers and maternal grandfathers; (3) there are sex differences in the anticipation of this role. Research questions addressed issues of age differences, lineage differences, grandparent-grandchild interactions during infancy and expectations for later interactions. Since previous research has attempted to differentiate four distinct grandparental role types, a replication of this research was performed. One-hundred-forty-six expectant grandparents completed questionnaires about grandparenthood. Eighty-six percent of the subjects continued participation after the birth In general, the sample was white, middle-class and well educated. The results of the study suggested that: (1) timing influenced eXpectations for interactions with the grandchild but was not related to difficulty perceiving oneself as a grandparent; (2) the significance of biological renewal followed same-sexed lineage patterns prenatally but conflicting results were obtained after the birth; (3) women began anticipating the grandparental role earlier than men and reported expectations for involvement when assessed prenatally. Men were more distant from the role until after the birth. Contrary to previous research, there were no age differences regarding styles of grandparenting. It is suggested that the developmental level of the grandchild is a more important determinant of grandparental style than the chronological age of the grandparent. Assumption of the grandparental role increased subjects} perceived age statuses. Maternal grandparents were more likely than paternal grandparents to see the grandchild first, but there were no lineage differences in terms of visits during infancy. Women were more likely to help the new family after the birth than were men. Replication of four types of grandparents occurred as predicted but the types could not be predicted from life style variables before the grandchild's birth and were not consistent longitudinally. Statistical arguments are presented which suggest a need to reconsider the value of grandparental types. THIS DISSERTATION IS DEDICATED TO MY MOTHER PATRICIA CAROL MCGREAL AND TO THE MEMORIES OF JOSEPH EUGENE MCGREAL CATHERINE MCGREAL DE ESTA MCFARLAND ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank the members of my dissertation committee, Dr. Hiram Fitzgerald, Dr. Ellen Strommen, Dr. Albert Rabin, and Dr. Jane Oyer for the support they gave me in conducting this research. I also would like to thank Dr. Elaine Donelson for her willingess to serve as a committee member during my oral defense. In particular, I wish to thank Hi Fitzgerald who served as the chairperson of my committee and as my advisor. Through the years of comps and courses, teaching and research, he has always been willing to answer the "one quick question" that I needed to ask. His assistance throughout my graduate career In”; always been appreciated though rarely acknowledged. The statistical advice provided by Dr. John Hunter and by the statistical consultants in the Department of Education was greatly appreciated. I also wish to express my gratitude to the Association for Shared Childbirth, the Expectant Parents' Organization and Lansing General Hospital for allowing me to attend their childbirth preparation classes in order to recruit subjects. ii I appreciate the willingness of all the grandparents in my study to share their family experiences with me. After sharing their experiences as "eXpectant" grandparents I found myself eagerly scanning the birth announcements awaiting the births of their new family members. I am grateful for having the opportunity to share my graduate years with Esther Dienstag. She is the sister I wanted throughout my childhood and finally found as an adult. I wish to thank my family, Patty, Tim, Michael, Natalie, and Patrick McGreal for their love and encouragement. I also wish to thank the family I've grown to love and call my own, Bud, Gary, Gloria, Frances and Marilou Miller, Gloria and Dale Vertz, and Ann Moore. Special thanks to my daughters, Erin and Kaiti. Without them my graduate career would have been shorter but not nearly as much fun! I thank them for the smiles and kisses that made the days of deleted computer files and lost data seem insignificant. Finally, I want to express my special appreciation for the love and support provided by Kerry Miller. He has continued to be as "conscious of my develOpment as he was of his own"! I thank him for sharing his life with me and look forward to all the "transitions" we have ahead of us. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES......... ....... ..........................vii LIST OF FIGURES..........................................ix Chapter I. INTRODUCTION Statement of the problem... ...... ........ ..... ...1 Review of the literature.........................5 Grandparents: An historial overview.......5 Research investigations on grandparenthood........................12 Grandfathers..............................18 Biological renewal: A significant aspect of grandparenthood?.............23 The grandparental role....................26 The timing of first grandparenthood.......32 Grandparenthood: A middle-age transition.............................41 Objective Of the studyOOOOOOOOOOC0.0.0.000000000uu II. METHODSOOOIOOOOOOOIOO0.00000000000000000.0.0.0..u7 Subject reoruitment..........................H7 Subjects.....................................H8 Instruments..................................52 Data Coding..................................5u Analyses.....................................56 iv III. RESULTSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOCOOOOOOOOOO... ..... .061 Expectant grandparents: An overview.........61 Anticipating grandparenthood..............61 Information about the pregnancy...........6fl Names.....................................66 Sex preferences...........................67 Plans to help during the neonatal period.................................67 Grandparenting during the newborn period: An overVieWOOOOO0....00.0.000000000000000068 Information about the birth...............68 Names........ ....... ......................71 Sex preferences...........................72 Help patterns in the neonatal period......73 Cluster anaIYSis 00000000000 0.0.0.00000000000073 Age differences..................... ......... 78 Expected timing of grandparenthood........87 Ideal age to become a grandparent.........89 LineageOOOOOO0..O0..0....00.0.0000000000000009u Anticipating grandparenthood..............9u Information about the pregnancy...........9u Information about the birth...............96 Names....................................1OO Sex preferences.... ...... ....... ....... ..100 Plans to help during the neonatal period................................101 Help patterns in the neonatal period.....103 Biological renewal.......................103 sex differenceSOOOOOOOOOO0.0.0.0000000000000106 Anticipating grandparenthood.............107 Information about the pregnancy..........109 Information about the birth..............110 Names....................................111 Sex preferences..........................112 Plans to help during the neonatal period................................113 Help patterns in the neonatal period.....11u Daily involvement with the grandchild....120 Grandparental role meaning types ..... .......121 Replication of Robertson's factor analySiSOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.0.0121 Role meaning types.......................128 Role behaviors..............................133 Satisfaction with grandparenthood...........135 IV. DISCUSSION The timing of first grandparenthood.........138 Biological renewal. ..... ....................1u2 Anticipating grandparenthood................1Hu Agedifferences.............................11H4 Lineage ..... ................................149 Sex differences.............................152 Grandparental role meaning types............156 Predicting role meaning types...............158 Role behaviors..............................159 Satisfaction with grandparenthood...........161 Summary..... .............. ...... ........... .162 APPENDICES Appendix A. Correspondence with subjects ........ ..... ...... 166 B. Sample questionnaires..........................169 C Questionnaire keys......... ........ ............215 D. Computer codebooks .......... . ...... . ..... ......225 E. ReliabilitieSOOOOO0....0.0.0.0...0.0.00.00000002u2 REFERENCES. ....................... ............. ........2H7 vi Table 13. 1M. 15. LIST OF TABLES Page Joint frequency table. Illustration of eXpected frequencies per cell. Question: Who told you that you would be a grandparent?.....58 When did you first imagine what it would be like to be a grandparent?.........................62 What was your first reaction when you found out you were going to be a grandparent?...............66 Learning of the grandchild's birth: Who told the grandparent of the baby's birth?..................69 Age of grandchild when first seen by grandparentOOOOOOOIOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 000000000000 69 Frequency of grandparent-grandchild visits........70 Intercorrelations between clusters....... ..... ....77 Subject distribution by sex and age.. ..... ........78 Prenatal frequencies for perceived age status.....86 Postnatal frequencies for perceived age status....86 Age differential: Comparison of chronological age and best age to become grandparent............93 EXpected age for grandparenthood by age differential0.0...OOOO0....00......0.0.0.00000000093 Lineage differences regarding the person by whom the subject was told of the pregnancy........95 Lineage differences in learning of the grandChild'S birthOOOOOOCOOOOOOOCOOOOOO0.0....0.0.98 Lineage differences in who told the grandparent Of the grandChild's birthOOOOOOOOOO ..... O. ........ 99 vii Table 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. Page Lineage differences in age of grandchild when first seen by grandparentn.n.n.u.u.u.u.99 Lineage and sex preferences.......................100 Lineage differences in plans to help out during the neonatal periodOOOOOOOOOOO0.0.0.00000000000000102 Lineage differences in whether plans to help out the eXpectant parents are dependent on the other set of grandparents' plans........................102 Sex differences in imagining oneself as a grandparentooOO...IO...00.0.0.0.0.0.00.00000000000108 Sex differences: Who told the subjects that they would be grandparents?.......................11O Subject's sex by preference for grandchild's seXOIOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0..OOOOOOOOOOOOOOCOOOICOOO0.00.112 Subject's sex by discussion of plans with Childrenoooooooooococo00000oooooooooooooooooooooo.11” Description of help patterns during neonatal periOdOOOOOOOOI0..OO...O00....0.00.00.00.000000000115 Role meaning items and factor loadings............122 Factor loadings: Principle components solution...126 Factor loadings: Principle factor solution.......127 Comparison of grandparental role meaning types....129 Consistent role types................. ..... .......131 Description of consistent role types..............132 Frequency of anticipated role behaviors...........13u viii Figure LIST OF FIGURES Page Prenatal eXpectations for timing of grandparenthoodOOOOO0.....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0000......81 Postnatal expectations for timing of grandparenthOOdOOO.IOOOOIOOOOIOOOOOOOOOOOO000......83 Prenatal perceived age status......................8N Postnatal perceived age status.....................85 Interaction between sex and lineage on feelings 0f biOIOgical renewaJ-OOOOOGOO.0.00.00.00.000000000105 sex patterns in lineage...........................105 ix CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Statement of the Problem Prior to this decade the grandparental role was one that generated considerable discussion but very little research. In 1980, Troll pointed out that there were less than a dozen studies in this area. Opinions on the subject have ranged from viewing the grandmother as a "pernicious influence"(n1grandchildren(Vollmer, 1937)1x>the notion that problems in families occur not because the family has lost its "moral fiber, " but because it has lost its grandmother (Mead, 1947). It is often suggested that since many roles are reduced or lost as a person ages, the grandparental role emerges as an important aspect of the aging person's life. Havighurst (195”), for example, states that it is the outstanding new role for older people and is often a source of great satisfaction. Richardson (1964, page 51) goes so far as to suggest that, "...being a grandparent can be a profession in itself. If wisely handled it can be the greatest happiness in the senior years." The purpose of the current study was to examine expectations for the grandparenting role before and after the birth of the first grandchild. In order to gain an understanding of the significance of the grandparental role to first-time grandparents, the following hypotheses were addressed: (1) (2) (3) Parents who become grandparents "off-time" in terms of their expected time-table for this event have greater difficulty considering themselves as grandparents than do parents who become grandparents "on-schedule"; Feelings of biological renewal and continuity through the grandchild are stronger for maternal grandmothers and paternal grandfathers than for paternal grandmothers and paternal grandfathers; Women begin to imagine themselves as grandparents at an earlier age than do men. Since the topic of grandparenthood has received limited research attention, several exploratory research questions also were addressed: (1) Do grandparents of different chronological ages report different expectations for the role? Research on experienced grandparents suggested that older grandparents assume a more formal style of grandparenting than do younger grandparents (Neugarten and Weinstein, 196“). However, other evidence suggested that the developmental level of the grandchild has implications for appropriate grandparental styles (Kahana sun! Kahana, 1970b). (2) (3) (‘4) Since in the present study the ages of the grandparents vary but the grandchildren are of the same age, it is possible to assess whether differences are found for grandparents of different ages. Does lineage affect expectations for grandparenting behaviors? Research has suggested that stronger relationships occur between maternal grandparents and their grandchildren than between paternal grandparents and their grandchildren (Kahana and Kahana, 19708). Therefore, the interactions between maternal and paternal grandparents and their grandchildren were compared to assess differences in behaviors due to lineage. Do grandparental expectations and interactions vary as a function of the grandparents' sex? It has been assumed that the grandparental role is a maternal role for men as well as for women (Boyd, 1969). However, men's attitudes concerning this role have received little attention. Can four distinct grandparental role meaning types be distinguished before the birth of the first grandchild? Robertson (1977) suggested that there are four types of grandparents that vary in their interactions with grandchildren. Replication of this conceptualization is important before it is accepted as a reliable way to investigate the meaning of grandparenthood. (5) (6) (7) Can grandparental role meaning types be predicted from life style variables before the grandchild is born? Research on experienced grandmothers and great- grandmothers suggested that life style variables differ for different types of grandparents (Robertson, 1977). However, the significance levels used to determine these relationships were liberal. Therefore, investigation of whether life style variables do predict role meaning types (when traditional levels of significance are used) is warranted. Do eXpeotant grandparents anticipate engaging in the same behaviors that are actually performed by experienced grandparents? What aspects of grandparenthood are considered to be the most and the least satisfying to men and women who have just assumed the grandparental role? Through the answers to these research questions more detailed hypotheses can be generated concerning the transition to grandparenthood. Review of the Literature The purpose of this review is to evaluate the literature on the grandparental role and thus provide a framework for an investigation of the transition into grandparenthood. One difficulty in the literature on grandparenting has involved the definition of a grandparent. Presumably one might consider a grandparent as someone whose child has given birth to (or adopted) a child. However, Fischer (1983) points out that there are four meanings commonly ascribed to the term: (1) an older person; (2) a particular generation (e.g., a generation might be called the "grandparent" generation in order to make intergenerational comparisons when the "parent" generation consists of adults in their uO's or 50's some of whom are also grandparents); (3) prescribed behaviors; (A) a social status in society. Throughout this paper the term will be used as a "relational term", which is Fisher's alternative to the above definitions. Thus a person is considered to be a grandparent through a relationship with a grandchild. Grandparents: An Historical Overview Early theoretical articles on grandparenthood often stressed the influence of the grandparent on problems in child-rearing. Abraham (1913/1955) reported that in his psychoanalytic treatment of neurotics and psychotics a theme that many patients would continue to return to involved their relationships with their grandparents. He reached the conclusion that "the Special emphasis given to the grandfather or grandmother was always rooted in a violent rejection of the father or the mother (Abraham, 1913/1955, page AM). Ferenczi (1927) supported Abraham's interpretation of a "grandfather complex". Rappaport noted that the grandchild's identification with a grandparent rather than with a parent led to "patterns of distorted, even grotesque, behavior .u" (Rappaport, 1958, page 518). Vollmer (1937) viewed the grandmother as a detrimental influence on grandchildren. He did not think that difficulties existed in particular grandmother-grandchild relationships due to the dynamics in the individual relationship. Instead he suggested that there is a problem in the very nature of the relationship: "The difficulty, then, arises not so much from the personality of the individual as ffimmi something inherent ix: being a grandmother" (Vollmer, 1937, page 379). IR) an article entitled, "Grandnma made Johnny delinquent", Strauss (1943) detailed the cases of two boys who had appeared hiJuvenile Court. He eXplained how the cause of each boy's behavior problem could be explained from '%a psychiatric point cM‘ view" involving the grandmothers. Bordon (1946) also discussed several cases in which she determined that the grandparents' involvement with the grandchild led to the behavior problems displayed by the child. In particular, she perceived the mother- adult daughter relationship as having a central role in the behavioral problems of the child. Although the majority of early articles concerning grandparenthood suggested that grandparents did have a negative influence on child development there were conflicting Opinions. During the same time period, for example, there were those who suggested that grandmothers shouldtnaused to prevent children from spending too much time with other family members who could negatively affect the child's development. The child's father was sometimes viewed as one of the negative influences! VonHentig (19MB- N6) cited rules concerning the mental health of the unemployed given in the book Morale by Dr. George K. Pratt: "It has been said that 'stability and mental health of the family under present social conditions depend to a considerable degree on father's not being at home too much. Since the unemployed can not be asked to stay away from home all the time, the same appeasing effect will be produced by removing the children to the grandmother's home" (VonHentig, 1945—A6, page 391). In this respect the grandmother was viewed as one who rescued the younger generation when unfortunate circumstances (such as those caused by the Depression and World War II) affect family life. LaBarre, Jessner and Ussery (1960) reviewed forty-one cases of children who had received treatment in the psychiatric unit of a university hospital. They found that the quality of the grandparent-grandchild relationship and the characteristics of the grandparent's personality were central in determining the role the grandparent played in the child's¢disturbance. Although negative influences on grandchildren such as those described above were found, the authors also indicated cases in which the presence of a grandmother exerted 23 positive influence cm: a grandchild. In discussing the early literature on grandparenting Hader (1965) points out that it can be divided into authors who feel that the influence of a grandparent on a grandchild is negative and those who feel that this influence is positive. Hader argued that it is the absence of a relationship with a grandparent that is deleterious to child develOpment. Actually the entire nature of this debate seems somewhat odd due to the vast differences between individuals performing the same role. For example, could one say that parental influence on a child is either negative or positive? The acceptance of this debate as a legitimate issue for discussion may have been due, therefore, to the assumption that the problems involved were inherent in grandparenthood regardless of individual personality deveIOpment. Streib and Thompson (1960) noted that the studies in this area were based primarily on clinical pOpulations and therefore generalization would not be possible without study of grandparent-grandchild relationships in the general pOpulation. It has also been suggested that many of the early studies were not typical in that grandparents were acting as surrogate parents to their grandchildren which isrun;a typical grandparental role (Kahana and Kahana, 1971). Although there appears to have been no resolution of this matter, discussion of grandparents in terms of children's behavior problems gradually waned fix: the 1960's as Imore systematic investigations began to be pursued. Despite the numerous theoretical Opinions on grandparenthood, there were few early research investigations on the subject. Noting that much of the literature pertaining to grandparents involved clinical evidence, Staples and Smith (195a) conducted a study comparing how mothers and grandmothers actually interacted with grandchildren in a non-clinical population. The research questions concerned child rearing practices. It was hypothesized that grandmothers would be stricter and more authoritative than would mothers. Each mother had a child under the agecn?12 living at home. Assessment was through the Wiley Scales for determining adult attitudes concerning child behaviors. These scales were used to obtain information on the women's attitudes toward general home standards, verbal standards, the amount of hostility that is considered appropriate to express, techniques for feeding and weaning children, approaches to thumbsucking and toilet-training, information given regarding sex, sex- role differences in apprOpriate rearing, and responses to crying. The results confirmed the authors' hypothesis that grandmothers would be stricter. Grandmothers who lived in separate households scored more permissively than did those living with the grandchildren. Mothers and grandmothers with higher educational levels tended in) be more permissive. There was a significant correlation between mother and daughter concerning these childrearing attitudes but only when they lived apart” The relationship between mothers' and daughters' attitudes was not significant when the women lived in the same household. Failure to find a relationship between the attitudes held by a woman and her daughter-in-law vuns not surprising. Recent research indicated that the attitudes of mother and grandmother were not related to whether they had joint or separate residences (Cohler and Grunebaum, 1981). However, the investigators pointed out that they did not use the same scale for measuring attitudes as was used by Staples and Smith (1954). Staples and Smith (1954) contributed to the understanding of grandparenthood by investigating the topic in a more objective manner than had been typical in the past. Duvall (1954) studied the grandmother role in the context of a study on in-laws. Her method of studying grandparenthood, however, was one that could lead to a positive bias in terms of the perception of the role. 10 There were two procedures for collecting data. First, data were collected from 1,337 subjects concerning which in-law relationship they felt was the most difficult one, what made it difficult, and what in-laws did to contribute to family harmony. Second, the content of 3,683 letters from a national radio contest ("December Bride" show) were analyzed by two social scientists to determine the predominant responses of peOple to the question, "Why I think mothers-in-law are wonderful peOple". Thus, although both samples were large, there is a methodological problem due to the biased sampling procedures followed. One of the categories used to code responses was "Mother-in—law is the children's grandmother". This was the predominant sentiment for very few subjects (7 of the 1,337 and 48 of the 3,683). But many subjects mentioned this as a secondary tOpic especially in terms of indicating that in- laws could always be counted (”1 to babysit their grandchildren. Duvall concluded that the responses in this area were so casual that the babysitting function of the grandmother was basically taken for granted by the young parents. Comments also showed that parents see grandparents as providing a fullness and breadth to a child's life that parents often don't provide. Parents viewed grandparents as those who could step in and rear the children in cases where the mother died. Very little 11 friction was indicated concerning the grandparent-parent- grandchild relationship but this would not be expected given the nature of data collection. The major contribution of this study does not appear to be in the results it obtained but rather in the research questions that Duvall proposed as a result of her findings. Among the questions she suggested for investigation were the following (many of uflflxfli are currently being considered): "Just what is eXpected of grandmother in American families? Who is a good grandmother and what does she do?". How much do grandparents feel 'out in the cold' as far as satisfying interactions with other members of their families is concerned? In what kinds of families is respect for elders practiced? In what ways?.HWhat roles may grandmothers play in the rearing of their grandchildren? Which are mutually satisfying and why? Which are stressful, and how may they be avoided?". Research Investigations of Grandparenthood Several of the questions posed by Duvall (1954) were investigated in a classic study conducted by Neugarten and Weinstein.(1964%. In this study 70 middle-class couples were studied to determine (1) their degree of comfort in the role; (2) the significance of the role to each person, and (3) the style with which each person enacted the role. The majority of the grandparents (59% of the grandmothers and 61% of the grandfathers) were found to express comfort 12 and pleasure in their role. Those who were not comfortable in the role seemed to indicate either that the role was foreign to their self-image, that they were in conflict with the parents over the grandchild, or that they were simply indifferenttHJany responsibility associated with the grandchildren. When determining the primary significance of grandparenthood for each subject, the authors found that the responses could be broken down into 5 categories: (1) Biological renewal and biological continuity with the future; (2) Emotional self fulfillment since the person can succeed in a new emotional role;(3) Acting in the role of a resource person; (4) Achieving vicariously through the grandchild; (5) Little significance attached to the role and the grandparent feels remote from the grandchildren. Neugarten and Weinstein also differentiated five major styles of grandparenting: (1) Formal grandparents who have a constant interest in the grandchildren but maintain their proper roles; (2) Funseeking grandparents who are informal and playful in their relationships with grandchildren; (3) Surrogate parents who assume caretaking responsibilities for their grandchildren; (4) Grandparents who are considered tolxathe reservoir of family wisdom and who dispense Special skills and resources; (5) Grandparents who are distant figures in the grandchildren's lives and who emerge for holidays but ordinarily have little contact with their grandchildren. 13 The formal, funseeking, and distant figure grandparents were found to be the most common styles. The formal style frequently occurred in those over age 65, whereas the funseeking and distant styles occurred more in younger grandparents (under age 65). Whether these differences were due to cohort effects, due to the age of the grandparent, or due to the ages of the grandchildren cannot be determined from these data. Crawford (1981) interviewed grandparents with 9-month-old grandchildren to determine which of these five styles would be endorsed. Once again the most common style endorsed by both grandfathers and grandmothers was that of the funseeker. But in contrasttx>the findings reported abowe(Neugarten and Weinstein, 1964), the style of teacher (similar to Neugarten and Weinstein's style of resource person) was the next most common style. The distant figure was the least common style. Boyd (1969) studied four-generation families and reported the roles played by the grandparents. She hypothesized that the role that would be valued in today's society would be different from what a valued grandparental role was in the past. Furthermore she suggested that at the current time the grandparent role is one that is earned by the person rather than automatically acquired. Her results showed that the valued grandparent was a person who listened and was available but withheld advice until it was requested. Boyd (1969) described the grandparent as an 14 indulgent person. She also concluded that the grandparental role has changed in today's society by becoming one that is maternal (both for grandmothers and grandfathers) and it also has become an extended parent role rather than a surrogate parent role. By this she meant that the grandparent functioned in terms of extending help to the parents rather than substituting for them. Thus grandparenthood was viewed as a supplement to the role of parent rather than as a competing role. Kahana and Coe (1969) investigated grandparents living in the community and those who were institutionalized to determine if the grandparent role takes on added importance as other roles are lost. Grandparents who still lived ha the community met with their grandchildren more often and had longer visits with grandchildren than did the institutionalized group. Those grandparents in the community, however, evidently had higher expectations for involvement since 47% of them reported that their grandchildren did not have enough time for them as compared to only 26% of the institutionalized group who reported this. Fifty-nine percent of the institutionalized group reported that there was little importance in being a grandparent and that the role had little salience for them. In comparison, only 21% of the community sample felt this way. Care must be taken in interpreting these results since most older people are not institutionalized. It 15 could be that institutionalization does not lead to disengagement from social roles, but instead that another factor is involved in both (for example, if a person has had many conflicts with her/his adult children this may mean that there is no alternative to institutionalization and that there has been no opportunity to establish an affectionate relationship with a grandchild). Information on the involvement of grandparents and their grandchildren has been obtained in research investigations that are concerned primarily with family relationships in later life rather than the grandparent- grandchild relationship in particular. In 1937, Morgan studied factors which contributed to the happiness of men and women who were receiving old age assistance. Forty- three percent of the subjects who had grandchildren reported seeing them very often. Many mentioned that helping out in the daily care of the grandchildren was one of their regular activities and that playing with grandchildren was a major form of recreation. Yet these grandparents did not seem to prefer a surrogate parent role. They felt the ideal arrangement was for an older person to live close enough to the children so that grandchildren could be seen frequently for "a little visit" but then the person had his or her own home and life to return to. 16 Research involving 252 family groups revealed that grandparents frequently developed family rituals that formed bonds with the grandchildren (Albrecht, 1962). Usually a couple was the center of the ritual rather than a single grandparent. Sussman (1954) found that family continuity was most frequently found when the young adults chose marriage partners from similar cultural, religious, educational and social-class backgrounds. In these cases the deveIOpment and maintenance of family rituals such as those found by Albrecht (1962) was possible. The grandparents provided help to the young parents, usually in the form of major purchases (house, car) and in return expected to receive affection and "the right to exercise the grandparental role" (Sussman, 1954, page 120). Exactly what rights this entailed was not specified. Research i1] other countries. has investigated grandparental attitudes. These studies indicate that there is considerable variation in the role cross-culturally. Townsend (1957) found that older people in England preferred living close to their children but not in the same home. The grandparents reported a high level of interaction with grandchildren. Sixty-three percent of the grandmothers and 20% of the grandfathers cared for their grandchildren in some way on a regular basis. This care included taking children to the park, preparing meals, baby-sitting, picking them up from school, etc. Rosenmayer and Kockeis (1962), however, found a much lower level of 17 involvement with only 10% of the Viennese grandparents in their study seeing a grandchild daily and 24% seeing a grandchild on :3 weekly basis. Research conducted in collective settlements in Israel showed that as older members began to withdraw from occupations in the collective, relationships with the grandchildren became a primary concern, especially for women (Talmon-Garber, 1962). Olsen (1976) studied three-generational Taiwanese families. The grandmothers had stricter attitudes toward childrearing than did the mothers, but they were more indulgent in 'terms of grandchildrean behaviors. Differences were found between widowed and non-widowed grandmothers such that the widows were more involved in nurturant aspects of childcare and the non-widows had more impact on their daughters-in-laws childrearing attitudes. Grandfathers In the past, the role of men in family relationships often received limited attention by researchers and theorists. To shed more light on the role of the grandfather the significance of the role to men as well as to women was considered in this investigation. Less than a decade ago fathers were still being conceptualized as "forgotten contributors to child deveIOpment" (Lamb, 1975). In recent years there has beenzasurge of interest in the role of fathers (for example, a bibliography on materials on fatherhood published in the United States 18 between 1975 and 1980 contains over 500 references!«- see Price-Bonham, Pittman, and Welch, 1981). Currently, however, it appears that the "grandfather is a forgotten man in research on intergenerational relations (Hagestad and Speicher, 1981). Therefore, men's expectations for the grandparental role and their interactions with infant grandchildren were considered in the current study. Clavan (1978) suggested that one reason grandfathers have been neglected by researchers can be attributed to the difficulties lower socio-economic middle-aged and older men have performing parenting activities. Furthermore, she suggested that by the time the grandfather retired and was able to devote time to grandparenting, the grandchildren no longer needed this type of extended parenting. Cavan (1962) suggested that men may have difficulty with the role at first because as a social role it seems maternal in nature. Boyd (1969) supported the view of a maternal role for grandfathers. However, Hagestad and Speicher (1981) argued that this assumption has not been tested. Another assumption concerning grandfathers that remains to be tested is that men are proud of grandchildren when they are middle-aged grandfathers but that they don't savor the role until they have retired (Cavan, 1962). A study of older men living in rural areas, however, revealed that men who were working full time actually had a greater number of face-to-face contacts with their grandchildren 19 than did men who were working part-time or who were retired (Powers and Brubaker, 1977). Deutscher (1962), commenting on socialization for the postparental period, suggested that Americans are socialized to think that change is good which leads to atnatter-of-fact acceptance of changes in the family. For example, he cited the following reaction of a father who was asked how it felt to become a grandfather: "like most things in my life, it's just a matter of course. Things can be eXpected, like you expect changes in jobs and expect children to be married. Natural events come afterward and you take those things as a matter of course" (Deutscher, 1962, page 511). Other writers, however, portray grandfatherhood in idealistic terms: "Why is it that when Mom comes into the room carrying the baby, the little tyke reaches out his arms for Grandpa to take him? Somehow or other he seems to feel the man's strength, and goes to him at once, snuggling contentedly in his arms without squirming or fussing, while his mother goes about her duties" (Richardson, 1964). Vollmer (1937) considered the grandmother to be a major problem in raising healthy children but suggested that grandfathers do not cause problems in childrearing because they remain remote and do not get involved with grandchildren. Hence they cause no problems. Anecdotal evidence suggested that men may show more active involvement with grandchildren than would be expected based on their interactions with their own 20 children. Fried and Stern (1948) interviewed 75 older persons in order to determine the situations with which aged family members must deal. Three women who reported that their husbands were more involved with grandchildren than they had ever been with their own children speculated that the grandfathers were attempting to provide the grandchildren with the attention that they had failed to give to their own children. Research conducted in England (Townsend, 1957) suggested that grandchildren become more important to men whose spouses have died. Although older men, in general, were less involved in regular care and activities with grandchildren than were grandmothers, several of those who were widowers expressed the fact that they derived extreme pleasure from the grandfather- grandchild relationship. One nu”) remarked that "."Playing with my grandchildren is my greatest pleasure" , while another stated that, "I'm a grandfather and that's the only goodness I get out of life" (Townsend, 1957, page 174). One of the few studies directly assessing the relationship between grandfathers and grandchildren was investigated from the point of view of the grandchildren. Pihlblad and Habenstein (1965) studied salient kinship patterns by asking high school seniors whether they knew their grandfathers' occupations. It was assumed that knowledge of the occupation meant a closer tie between grandfather and grandchild. Their sample was equally 21 composed of boys and girls; 10% of the subjects were black, 90% of the subjects were white. Although there were no sex differences in knowledge of grandfathers' occupations there were racial differences: 49% of the white students and 21% of the black students knew the occupations of both grandfathers; 32% of the white students and 29% of the black students knew the occupation of one grandfather; 19% of the white students and 50% of the black students did not know either grandfather's occupation. When the authors looked at which grandfather‘s occupation was known (when only one was reported) 61%(H?the white students knew the paternal grandfather's occupation whereas 70% of the blacks knew the maternal grandfather's occupation. Pihlblad and Habenstein also considered the social class of the students and found that knowledge of the occupation of one grandparent was not related to social class but that knowing the occupations of both grandfathers decreased from the high social class to the middle and then to the lower social class. The results of this study suggest that findings concerning lineage patterns have a strong likelihood of being influenced by race and thus the race of the sample should be considered when generalizing results. Grandfathers have proven to be a difficult group to study even for those who have an interest in including them in studies of the family life cycle. Feldman, Biringen, and Nash (1981) assessed fluctuations in sex differences for several life stages: adolescents, single adults, 22 married-childless, eXpectant parents, young parents, mature parents, empty nesters and grandparents. Although most of their groups had a high acceptance rate for participation in the study (80-95%) the acceptance rate for the grandfathers was only 50%. In the current study efforts were made to include enough men that an analysis of the significance of the grandfather role would be possible. In this way the assumption that women have a wider range of meaningful grandparental activities associated with their role than do men (Thompson and Streib, 1962) can be considered. Biological Renewal: A Significant Aspect of Grandparenthood? "No one wants to be a footstep planted in the sand near the rushing sea, a footprint to be washed away into eternal waters, 3 sign seen for an instant and then erased forever" (B.BL Smith, 1973, page 27). Is the continuity of the family line through a grandchild viewed by grandparents as significant? In 1937, Morgan asked older individuals listed in Who's Who what they felt were the chief causes of unhappiness in the aged. A noted geneticist responded that lack of descendants led many to feel futile in (HA! age. Benedek (1970) discussed the depression and frustration experienced by those whose married children did not have children. Based on her psychoanalysis of these cases she hypothesized that the reason for the depression may be "the wish to survive in 23 the grandchild" (Benedek, 1970, page 200). Results from a study on family relationships in which subjects were encouraged to free associate revealed that grandparents were "pleased to see their physical lives continued in their grandchildren, so to speak.”" (Fried and Stern, 1948). Neugarten and Weinstein (1964) reported that 42% of the grandmothers and 23% of the grandfathers in their study stressed biological renewal as of primary significance in being a grandparent. This finding suggested that biological renewal may be more important to women than to men. This view was supported by research by Crawford (1981) in which 42% of the women and 26% of the men who were interviewed when their first grandchild was 9-months- old stated that the most significant aspect of grandparenthood was bdological renewal. However, in considering these data it is necessary to note that in both studies (Neugarten & Weinstein, 1964 and Crawford, 1981) more of tflua grandparental couples were 'maternal grandparents than paternal grandparents. In Neugarten and Weinstein's research 1H5 of the grandparental couples were maternal whereas 24 of the couples were related to the grandchild through their son. In Crawford's study 68 of the grandparents were maternal grandparents and only 39 were paternal grandparents. Furthermore, Crawford reported that 18 subjects were not asked the question about 24 biological renewal due to difficulties in the interview. Thus, the exact number (N? maternal and paternal grandparents responding to the question cannot be determined. An alternate interpretation to the view that women emphasize the importance of biological renewal through the grandchildren more than do men would be that this significance is also affected by lineage. Since the samples in the studies by Neugarten and Weinstein (1964) and Crawford (1981) both consisted cu? more maternal grandparents than paternal grandparents the obtained sex differences may have been due to the fact that women perceived biological continuity through their daughters whereas men tended to view it as occurring through their sons. Research conducted with three generations of 148 families revealed that there were closer bonds between paternal grandfathers and their grandsons and between maternal grandmothers and their granddaughters (Hagestad and Speicher, 1981). Further support for this interpretation comes from the fact that similarity between one's children and one's grandchildren was indicated by 58% of the maternal grandmothers but only 20% of the paternal grandmothers in a study of family structure (Kahana and Kahana, 1969). This study also found that paternal grandfathers indicated more involvement with grandchildren than did maternal grandfathers. Results conflicting with those of Neugarten and Weinstein (1964) were found in a 25 study of grandparents with divorced children. It was reported that grandparents with divorced sons reported more feelings of continuity through their grandchildren than did the grandparents with divorced daughters (Kahana and Kahana, 1980). In the current study, perception of biological renewal and continuity of the family ldxue were obtained from maternal and paternal grandparents in order to investigate this issue. Whether other differences in the perception of grandparenthood also occur as a result of lineage was considered in the current study. Parsons (1943) suggested that the terminology used in American society to describe grandparents reflects the kinship structure. That is, whether one is on the paternal side of the family or on the maternal side of the family, the person is called a "grandparentW. This lack of distinction in referring to lineage implies an equality between grandparents. Others, however, do suggest stronger relationships based on lineage (Kahana and Kahana, 1970a), especially between mothers and daughters (Kahana and Kahana, 1969). The Grandparental Role Breen (1960, page 158) pointed out that throughout pe0ple's lives they assume roles that "reflect the social judgments of the larger society within which the individual lives and acts". Roles have been defined as eXpectations that are associated with certain positions in society 26 (Wahl, 1968). A certain amount of flexibility is involved in the assumption of new roles. Donahue, Orbach, and Pollak (1960) noted that "a given role is characterized by a series of potentially varying interpretations of the appropriate role behavior from the different perspectives and behavioral sources of expectations of the others in the functional situations wherein the role is performed". Although some have suggested that American society has no social role for older individuals:(eg, Clavan and Vatter, 1972) other authors (eg, Havighurst, 1954; Breen, 1960), have emphasized the grandparental role as appropriate in the later years. Although this role has usually been discussed as one of old age, the assumption of the role is usually a middle-aged occurrence as will be noted below. Winch (1963) suggested that the grandparental role is one marked by ambivalence since pe0ple have both the satisfaction of being released from parental responsibility now that the children have become adults and yet they also must deal with the fact that they have begun their "declining years". Perhaps an even more salient reason suggesting ambivalence in this role would be the fact that there are not clear eXpectations for behaviors as a grandparent. Friedmann (1960), in a discussion of how role expectations have changed in the past century, pointed out that there has been a loss of functioning as an elder and as an instructor to the young. However, he also emphasized 27 the fact that a century ago there actually were few families that included a grandparent in their home. It has been noted that the functions involved in the grandparental role represent reductions from the parental functions since the grandparent is not expected to socialize the young children and in this respect the new role may not be valued as highly by society (Riley, Foner, Hess, and Toby, 1969). Kahana and Kahana (1971) suggested that when the person has been able to shape the grandparental role, more enjoyment has been derived from it. This is12a role, however, that is defined in many respects, by the young parents rather than the grandparent (Thompson and Streib, 1962; Robertson, 1975). If discrepancies are found between persons'conceptualizationcfi‘the grandparental role and their actual role behaviors, therefore, it may be due to the range Of behaviors considered apprOpriate by the grandchild's parents. In her study of widowhood, Lopata (1973) found that grandmothers who were close to their grandchildren usually had many opportunities to see the grandchildren alone, without the parents. The mediational role Of the parents also may explain, in part, why involvement with friends has been found to be more closely related to life satisfaction in grandparents than has involvement with grandchildren (Wood and Robertson, 1976). The individual has more Opportunity to shape the role of friend than the role of grandparent. 28 Grandparental Role-Meaning Types. The role of the grandparent has been examined in order to develop a typology of grandparents (Wood and Robertson, 1976; Robertson, 1977). Two dimensions were considered in this typology: a personal dimension (eXpectations toward grandparenthood that stem from personal forces) and a social dimension (expectations about grandparenthood that are determined by societyL. High and low scoring Of each dimension led to 4 role-meaning types: Apportioned, Symbolic, Individualized, Remote. Robertson (1977) found that these role types could be predicted by various life style variables such as education and life satisfaction. Robertson's sample included grandmothers and great- grandmothers. Therefore, there was considerable variation in terms of subjects' experiences as grandmothers. Since Robertson stated that "Role meaning is a product Of anticipatory socialization, and an individual assumes the grandparent role with a host of preconceived attitudes and expectations regarding the role" (Robertson, 1977, page 167), it was felt that a replication of Robertson's analysis using subjects assuming the grandparental role would yield information on the usefulness of her scale for other groups of grandparents. Therefore, an additional purpose of the current investigation was to replicate the factor analysis used1x>develop Robertsonfistypology and, if replicated, to determine if the grandparental role 29 meaning types could be predicted from life style variables before the birth of the first grandchild. Robertson determined the four roles types through factor analytic techniques. After administering 20 Likert- type items tO 125 grandmothers, a factor analysis was performed. From this initial pool of items, 12 items were found to be significant at the .05 level. Six items loaded high on Factor ll which Robertson labelled the Social Dimension. The other six items loaded high on Factor II, which was called the Personal Dimension. Subjects were assigned scores for each Of the 12 items creating a personal dimension score andaasocial dimension score. A mean split was used to dichotomize subjects into high scorers and low scorers for each dimension. Thus the four groups were created. Robertson studied six life style variables (age of grandmother, education, number Of grandchildren, satisfaction \with life, friendship associations, and community associations) in order to see if the grandparental role meaning types were associated with particular life styles. One difficulty with this study has been the liberal setting of significance levels. Within the article each type Of grandparent is described in terms Of characteristic behaviors on each of these life style variables. Confidence in the value Of these comparisons is lessened, however, when one considers the significance levels. Only two Of the six variables, education and 30 satisfaction with life, would be considered significant at the traditional level of significance (probability = .05). Therefore, in the current study, conservative levels Of significance were used to determine whether there were indeed, differences in life style variables among the four grandparental role meaning types. In addition to her research on grandmothers, Robertson also collected information from 132 grandfathers in order to determine their role meaning types (Wood and Robertson, 1976). Once the men were divided into four groups their scores on several life style variables were combined with the grandmothers' scores reported above. For the entire group Of grandparents four variables were found to be significantly different: the person's age, his or her educational level, the number Of grandchildren, and the number Of activities the grandparent engaged in with the grandchildren. However, it is difficult to evaluate these data due tO the fact that levels for determining the significance Of these variables were not reported. Grandparental Role Behaviors. One of the research questions addressed in the current study concerned whether expectant grandparents anticipated engaging in the behaviors that experienced grandparents perform. Robertson (1977) studied role behaviors which grandmothers reported engaging in with grandchildren. The behaviors that the majority Of the experienced grandmothers reported included 31 providing gifts, babysitting the grandchild, engaging in home—recreational activities and drOpping in to visit the grandchild. A comparison was made to determine whether expectant grandparents anticipated engaging fill these behaviors. The Timing Of First Grandparenthood Another aspect Of grandparenthood to be considered concerns when :3 person becomes a grandparent. It was hypothesized that the timing of grandparenthood influences persons'reactions to this event. In particular, parents who become grandparents "Off-time" were hypothesized to have greater difficulty assuming this role. In determining the significance of grandparenthood, Neugarten and Weinstein (1964) found that 27% Of the grandmothers and 29% Of the grandfathers attached little significance to the role. Research would be useful to determine whether this remoteness is due tO role strain (as may occur, for example, if someone becomes a grandparent at an earlier age than had been anticipated) or if it evolved after conflicts with the parents and nonrewarding interactions with the grandchild. It has been noted that American society has definite expectations that are based on chronological age for the assumption of roles (Tibbitts, 1960; Neugarten and Datan, 1973). Neugarten, Moore and Lowe (1965) called this a "prescriptive timetable for the ordering Of major life 32 events". Individuals in our society are aware of whether they are early, on-time, or late in regard to this timetable (Neugarten and Datan, 1973). In research conducted with a middle-aged sample, subjects reported that the best ages to become grandparents were from 45 to 50 years Of age (Neugarten, Moore and Lowe, 1965). Other writers have speculated that these ages are actually too early to assume this role. Cavan (1962) suggested that grandparent self-conceptions become part cfi‘ people's personalities when the persons reach theirEHPs and early 60's. Waller and Hill (1951) felt that when parents are in their late 40's they may still consider the role inappropriate and resist it because it is an overt sign of their chronological maturity. It also has been noted that the current middle—aged cohort is overcommitted in terms Of roles (Hess and Waring, 1978) which may also have an effect on their feelings toward assuming another role. However, life events, rather than chronological age, may be the relevant factors in terms of a person's expectations for timing of grandparenthood. Neugarten and Weinstein (1964) suggested that there is a preparatory period for grandparenthood that occurs (especially for women) before children are married. During this time peOple begin to visualize themselves as grandparents. Mead (1970) also considered the marriage of one's child to be a key time to begin conceptualizing oneself as a grandparent. She pointed out that at that time one becomes aware of the 33 new elements that will be brought into the line Of descent through the daughter- or son-in-law. Richardson (1964) stated that a joy to many parents at the marriage Of their children is the anticipation Of grandchildren. The concept of an expected life-history (Bortner, 1978/79)iJsuseful in considering the significance of the timing Of grandparenthood. An expected life—history is a scenario that most adults develOp concerning their expectations for the future. Some peOple may have a very general expected life-history while others have a minutely detailed plan for the future. Culture, social class, family background, career and individual family events all may play a role in the develOpment Of the expected life- history. Family life histories are, by their very nature, intertwined. A child has tremendous impact on the lives Of the parents. The parents envision a life-history for their children. One part Of this scenario for one's children's future would most likely include marriage and children. This, in turn, makes grandchildren part of the expected life—history Of the parents. As family life cycle changes take place many parents may find that they do not become grandparents at the time they anticipated in their expected life-histories. It would be useful to determine when parents begin to imagine themselves as grandparents to clarify issues involved in the timing of becoming a grandparent. 34 F. Cottrell (1960) discussed family Obligations in low-energy versus high-energy societies. A low energy society was defined as one that secured its energy from plants, animals and humans (lesstflunmS horsepower-hours per day per person). A.high energy society used the above sources Of energy plus additional sources (more than 5 horsepower-hours per day per person). Cottrell pointed out that in low-energy societies decisions concerning conception do not belong exclusively to the husband and wife. In some societies a‘woman may not be accepted as a member of a man's family until she has already borne his child (and in some cases the child must be a male!). Other groups require young couples to practice sexual intercourse taboos and infanticide in order to decrease fertility during periods when the Older family members decide that there are already enough peOple attempting to live Off the land. In high-energy, technological societies, however, Older parents no longer can determine when a couple has children and how many they will have. Furthermore, the younger generation has come to expect complete fertility control due to technological advances in contraception (Poston and Gotard, 1977). Thus, in American society, the timing Of grandparenthood is out of the control Of the grandparents themselves. When the timing of grandparenthood does not occur according to society's timetable or within the bounds Of the person's eXpected life history, there may be more 35 difficulty for the person assuming this role. When an event occurs earlier than is anticipated then one is out Of phase (Schneidman, 1967) and the ability to derive satisfaction from the role may be reduced. Late assumption Of the role also may cause difficulties especially if the person no longer has the health to perform those role behaviors that had been anticipated as part Of the grandparental role. Changes in the family life cycle reveal that there are discontinuities from one generation to the next. These changes may make it difficult to anticipate when various life events will occur. Two striking achievements Of civilization have been the lengthening Of the family life cycle and the changes to the adult life cycle (Tibbitts, 1965). By reviewing what changes the family life cycle has undergone in this century it may be possible to determine what expectations individuals may have formed concerning their futures. Of particular interest are the marriage and birth patterns during; the 1950's because today's grandparents may have formed their eXpectations during these years as they were becoming parents. Glick (1955, 1977) found several trends in the family life cycle that have implications for Older members Of the family. In reviewing the time period from 18801x>1950, Glick found that the average median age at first marriage for women was 20.9 years. The lowest level was found during the 1950's 36 when the median age was 20.0 years. Men's ages at first marriage declined from 25.4 years Of age in 1880 to 23.6 years in 1950. Those marrying in the 1950's, therefore, were marrying earlier than the generations before them. In addition to earlier marriages, couples Of this decade also had a shorter interval between marriage and the birth of the first child. The median age of a woman giving birth to her first child has ranged from 21.5 years in the 1950's to 23.5 years in the 1930's. Women during the 1950's not only had their first child earlier, they also stopped having children earlier than had the generations before them. In fact, Glick estimated that halfcfi‘these women had borne their last child by the time they were 26-years-Old. This reduction in the span Of the childbearing years has made family roles more distinct. That is, the likelihood that a person would become a grandparent while still parenting young children has been greatly reduced. In this respect the grandparent role has become more distinct. Longer life eXpectancies and changes in the family life cycle indicate a longer postparental period for many couples (GliCk, 1955, 1977). The average married couple in 1890 did not survive jointly to see their last child married. By 1950 the average couple could eXpect 41 years Of married life before one died (as compared to 31 years in 1890) and the last 14 of these years would be postparental years when the couple is once again alone. These postparental years represent the possibility of extended 37 grandparent-grandchild interactions which were not possible a century ago. In fact, Hareven (1977) noted that it has only been in the twentieth century that this meaningful overlap in the lives of grandparents and their grandchildren has occurred. Those who have considered this "empty nest" period as a time of strain for the couple have suggested that grandparenthood eases the strain (Blau, 1973). However, the strain of this period is questionable. Treas and Bengtson (1982) stated that these postparental years may be viewed in a more positive manner than in the past due to the increased affluence in American society. Today, parents may be relieved because they have lost a dependent; in the past parents Often lost a contributor to the family's income (whether in terms Of actual earnings or as a source Of farm labor) when a child left. This positive view of the postparental years has been supported by research on marital satisfaction during this period. After reviewing studies on the relationship between marital satisfaction auui family transitions, Rollins and Galligan (1978, page 81) concluded that "general marital satisfaction decreases during marriage simultaneously with the arrival and develOpment of the Oldest child in the family until about adolescence, and then as children mature and leave home, it increases". Changes in the family life cycle affect roles Older peOple can assume. These life cycle changes occur quickly 38 when compared to the changes in societal values. Habits and values are slow to change which places large numbers Of peOple in ambiguous positions. This is the case with many Older peOple of today (Tibbitts, 1965). Havighurst (1954) pointed out that appropriate roles change with age. As some roles are reduced (for example: parent, spouse, employee) other roles are intensified (for example: citizen, neighbor, friend). Role categorizations by age are useful tn) a society in that they "perform an 'integrative function' to the extent that they lend continuity through the recruitment and training of younger persons in the major institutional roles to which they will succeed" (Friedmann, 1960, page 121). Yet in our discontinuous society these role categorizations may not be applicable to many individuals. This places these individuals in a difficult situation since society has a lag in providing alternate role expectations. According to Tibbitts (1960), middle age and Old age are both functions determined, in part, by how society defines age itself and by the assignment Of various roles to different age categories. A traditional role assigned to Older peOple has Often been that Of grandparent but the timing of this role is dependent on changes in the family life cycle. Although it has been predicted that the 4 and 5 generation family will become the norm by the year 2000 (Beattie, 1975) it seems that there may actually be many alternate family styles by that year due to the increased acceptance 39 Of delayed parenthood. Considerable variation in timing of the transition to grandparenthood was expected in the current study due to present trends which have shown an increase in the median age at first marriage and an increase in the number of first births to women in their late 20's and 30's (O'Rand and Henretta, 1982). Research on the timing Of grandparenthood has been conducted in an indirect manner in the context Of adolescent pregnancy. Bacon (1974) discussed the stressful effects of accelerated role transitions in terms Of teenage mothers who deviated from the prescribed timetable for becoming a parent. This event also would lead to accelerated transition to grandparenthood on the part of the adolescent's parents since it is at variance with society's norms and probably with the person's eXpected life history. According to Neugarten and Datan (1973, page 62) age deviancy in terms Of being "Off-time" is "always Of psychological significance to the individual". Few studies inthis area focus directlycnmthe grandparents. Mothers have been shown tx> react to their teenage daughter's pregnancy as a crisis which indicates their own inadequacy as parents (Bryan-Logan and Dancy, 1974). The mothers usually are themselves young and have begun to anticipate freedom from the reSponsibilities Of their own children (E. W. Smith, 1975). When the baby is born there is Often 40 conflict because the adolescent mother does not assume full responsibility Of the baby, leaving the grandmother in the position where it. is inappropriate tn) assume the traditional mother role or the traditional grandmother role (Bryan-Logan and Dancy, 1974). The grandmother role is not welcomed because it Ineans the assumption cM‘ more responsibility for her child and grandchild, as well as implying her own aging process (Smith, 1975). Research in this area has emphasized the assumption Of the grandmother roletnrthe maternal grandmother because she has traditionally provided the familial support to the adolescent mother who is unmarried. Comparable research on the grandfather and the paternal grandparents has not been conducted. The timing of grandparenthood in terms of late transition to the role also has not been a focus Of research. Grandparenthood: A Middle-Aged Transition The transition to grandparenthood is usually a middle- aged occurrence. However, as Troll (1971, page 276) pointed out, when the focus has been on the nuclear family "all grandparents tend in) be described as Old". The stereotype of a grandparent is Often of a person who is out of the mainstream of contemporary life and out-of—date (Mead, 1966). Even when they do appear in children's literature and films they are generally one-dimensional characters (Davis, 1977). In contrast to this stereotype 41 of an aged person, most parents become grandparents long before they are aged. In pointing out the differences between the stereotypical grandmother and a more accurate version Brandt (1968, page 26) emphasized that the role is "one Of the rich bonuses of middle age". Borland (1978) cited a need for research which focuses on middle age. lxxparticular she suggested that research should be designed to determine the "biological, psychological, and social transition points in the adult years where middle age begins and ends" (Borland, 1978). In discussing the awareness Of middle age, Neugarten (1968) suggested that women defined their status in terms Of family events whereas men used career and health events in their definitions of middle age. Although commonly considered a role of the aged, becoming a grandparent may actually be one of these transition points which emphasizes a beginning to middle age. It has been noted that the birth of the first grandchild denotes another social age with accompanying alterations in relationships with others (Neugarten and Datan, 1973). Therefore, in the current investigation the subject's perceptions of age status was assessed before and after the birth of the first grandchild. Tibbitts (1965) pointed out that in many other societies there were only 3 stages in life: childhood, adulthood and Old age. Today, he suggests, there are two 42 additional stages that have been inserted before Old age: middle age and late maturity; Neugarten and Datan (1973) pointed out how historical changes such as the delineation of a stage Of middle age directly influences the life cycle of the individual. Jackson (1974) studied the perceptions of middle age as held by adults between 75 and 85 years of age. It was found that 66% felt that middle age occurred from 40 to 50 with women's responses clustering at the younger end Of the range. Drevenstedt (1976), however, found in a sample of senior citizens and university students that women tended to judge the transition to middle-age as occurring later then did men. Given the ambiguity that exists utilizing only chronological age as a means of classification, it may be useful to also consider the roles that the person is currently enacting in order to determine their perception Of their own age classification. 43 Objective Of the Study The objective of this study was to examine the impact Of grandparenthood on grandparents at the time Of their transition to grandparenthood. In order to assess this transition comparisons were made in prenatal and postnatal conceptualizations Of grandparenthood among first—time grandparents. Hypothesis 1. Hypothesis 2. Hypothesis 3. The following hypotheses were investigated: Individuals who become grandparents "Off- time", will have greater difficulty considering themselves as grandparents than those individual becoming grandparents "on-schedule". Feelings of biological renewal will be indicated more strongly for maternal grandmothers and for paternal grandfathers than for paternal grandmothers and for maternal grandfathers. Women begin tn) imagine themselves as grandparents at an earlier age than do men . 44 In addition, the following research questions were addressed: 1. Are there age differences in terms of response to grandparenthood such that those Of different ages report different expectations? Although some researchers have found that age of the grandparents affects their grandparenting roles (eg, Robertson, 1977), others have suggested that the age differences may be a result of the developmental level Of the grandchild (Kahana and Kahana, 1970b). These contrasting views can be compared by assessing whether age differences exist before the first grandchild's birth. Are there lineage differences in expectations for grandparenting behaviors and in the significance Of grandparenthood?) Anecdotal evidence suggests that maternal grandparents have priority over the paternal grandparents in caring for the new parents and grandchild. Whether there is more information flow through the maternal side also will be investigated. Are there sex differences in regard to expectations for grandparenthood? Anecdotal evidence would suggest differences in terms of whether grandfathers and grandmothers plan to help out at the time Of birth. 45 Whether men and women are equally likely tO be aware Of their children's plans for a family also will be explored. Can four distinct grandparental role meaning types be distinguished through a replication Of Robertson's (1977) factor analytic techniques? If the above replication occurs as predicted, can the grandparental role meaning types be predicted from life style variables before the birth of the first grandchild? Do expectant grandparents anticipate engaging in role behaviors that experienced grandparents report engaging in? What aspects of grandparenthood are considered the most satisfying after the first grandchild's birth? What aspects are considered to In: the least satisfying? Since the tOpic of grandparenthood has received limited research attention, several aspects of this research are exploratory in nature. It.is hOped that by addressing these issues, it will be possible to generate further hypotheses concerning the grandparental role. us CHAPTER 2 METHODS Subject Recruitment Due to the need to Obtain subjects who were in the process of becoming grandparents, the majority Of subjects were recruited from childbirth preparation classes. The Association for Shared Childbirth and the EXpectant Parents'CWganization allowed the investigator to attend their prenatal classes. .Aftertfluestudy was explained to the expectant parents they were requested to provide the names and addresses of their parents. The expectant parents were assured that the involvement Of their own parents in the study would be strictly voluntary. If an address was provided then a questionnaire was mailed to the eXpectant grandparents with a letter inviting them to participate. Other participants were recruited from a newspaper article describing the study. Readers made telephone contact with the the department Of psychology. They were then contacted by the investigator who explained the study, and if they were still interested, mailed them a questionnaire. Finally, business—reply postcards were placed in the Obstetricians' Offices so that pregnant women 47 could pass them on to their parents or in-laws. When a postcard was received the eXpectant grandparents were mailed a letter inviting them to participate in the study. Samples of the letters mailed to prospective subjects can be found in Appendix A. Informed consent forms (also found in Appendix A) were mailed to and returned by subjects with their questionnaires in business—reply envelOpes. Subjects Full Sample. Sixty-seven percent Of the 218 eXpectant grandparents invited to participate in the study agreed to do so. The Full Sample included 146 parents (66 men, 80 women) whose adult children were eXpecting the first grandchild. Subjects ranged in age from 27- to 77-years— Old (§==53). 90% were married, 7% were divorced, and 1% were widowed (2% Of the subjects did not answer this question). Six of the subjects were becoming step— grandparents. There was considerable variation in the ages of the subjects' children. In fact, some of the eXpectant parents (ages 17 to 39) were Older than some of the expectant grandparents in the study! The majority Of grandparents (80%) reported a family income of over $20,000. The average level Of education was 14 years (range: 8 to 20 years, mode: 12 years). There were 80 maternal grandparents, 63 paternal grandparents, and 3 grandparents who would soon become maternal and paternal grandparents. 48 Selected Sample. In several cases it was deemed apprOpriate to utilize a subsample Of the subjects for statistical analysis. This subsample (referred to hereafter as the Selected Sample) consisted of 132 respondents. Excluded from the Selected Sample were: those who lived outside the United States; those who were becoming grandparents through a step-child; those whose children - the expectant parents - were single at the time the grandparent was contacted. The expectant grandfathers in the Selected Sample (n = 61) ranged in age from 40- to 77-years-old (E = 53 years). Ages Of the eXpectant grandmothers (n = 71) ranged from 37 to 71 years (I: = 53). Twenty-eight of the men and 30 Of the women were becoming paternal grandparents; 32 of the men and 40 Of the women were becoming maternal grandparents. One couple reported that since a son and a daughter both were eXpecting children at the same time they were about to become maternal and paternal grandparents. The number Of children in the subjects' families ranged from 1 to 8 (R = 3.1; mode = 3.0). Their youngest children ranged in age from 12 to 36 years (i = 22); age Of the oldest child ranged from 20 to 40 years (§'= 28.2). Women's ages at the time of marriage ranged from 14 tO 38 years (i = 21.8; mode = 19); mens' ages at marriage ranged from 18 to 34 years (i = 23.9; mode = 23). Ages at the time of the birth Of the first child ranged from 16 to 39 years for the women (i = 49 24.4) and from 19 to 44 years for the men (56 = 26.2). There was also considerable variation in terms Of the educational level Of the subjects. The highest grade completed by the women ranged from 9 to 20 (x' : 13.8; mode = 12); the highest grade completed by the men ranged from 8 to 20 (i = 14.8; mode = 12). or the 129 subjects who provided information on their families' income the majority reported incomes over $20,000. per year whereas only 15.5% earned less than $20,000. The majority Of the subjects reported that they were white h1=:100), 2 were black, 1 was hispanic, and 4 reported other racial or ethnic categories (e.g., American). Twenty-five subjects declined to respond to this question. Longitudinal Full Sample. Subjects who completed a prenatal questionnaire were mailed a letter and a postnatal questionnaire requesting their continued participation after the birth of their first grandchild. Eighty—six percent of the subjects in the Full Sample continued participation in the project by returning the postnatal questionnaire. (One grandmother indicated that her grandchild was stillborn and that was the reason she was drOpping out Of the study). These 126 subjects are referred to as the Longitudinal Full Sample. This group of 70 grandmothers and 56 grandfathers ranged in age from 27 to 77 years (i = 54). Of these, 120 were white, 1 was black, 1 was hispanic, 2 were oriental and 1 reported other racial categories (1 declined to answer). A more detailed 50 assessment of subject income was Obtained on the postnatal questionnaire than on the prenatal questionnaire. It revealed the following income distribution: Under $5,000 n = 4 3.2% $5,000 - $10,000 n = 1 .8% $10,000 - $15,000 n = 9 7.1% $15,000 - $20,000 n = 7 5.6% $20,000 - $25,000 n = 12 9.5% $25,000 - $30,000 n = 20 15.9% $30,000 - $35,000 :1 = 16 12.7% $35,000 - $40,000 n = 15 11.9% Over $40,000 n = 34 27.0% NO Response n = 8 6.3% All but one cfi‘ the grandparents indicated their current marital status: 114 were married; 8 were divorced; 2 were widowed; 1 was separated. Most of the grandparents indicated that the parents Of their grandchild were married (n = 123); one indicated that the parent was single; one indicated that the grandchild's parents had an "Other" type Of relationship; one grandparent declined to answer this question. The grandchildren of the subjects in the study were born between the summer of 1982 and the summer of 1983. Subjects were once again asked how far they lived from their grandchild. It was found that 342% lived in the same 51 household, 4.8% lived within walking distance, 56.3% lived within daily driving distance, 27% lived over 100 miles away and 7.9% lived over 1,000 miles away. Longitudinal Selected Sample. A Longitudinal Selected Sample (LSS) was formed by excluding from the Longitudinal Full Sample those subjects who lived outside the United States, those who were step-grandparents, and those who reported that the parent of their grandchild was single. The 117 subjects comprising this sample were 52 men and 65 women. They ranged in age from 37 to 77 (3: = 54). 90.6% were married, 6% were divorced; 1.7% were widowed, and .9% were separated L9% declined to answer). NO grandparent in the LSS reported a grandchild living in the same household, 5.1% lived within walking distance, 59.8% lived within daily driving distance, 28.2% lived over 100 miles away and 6.0% lived over 1,000 miles away. Instruments Research was conducted through paper-and-pencil questionnaires (see Appendix B for sample questionnaires). Although most of the expectant parents lived within the immediate vicinity i1; was anticipated that Inany grandparents might live at distances that would make interviews difficult. This indeed proved to be the case as indicated in the description Of subjects above. The questionnaire was constructed using items based on: themes of biological renewal, serving as a resource 52 person, emotional self-fulfillment, achieving vicariously through a grandchild, and remoteness from the grandparental role (based on Neugarten and Weinstein's categorization Of the significance Of the role); themes concerning formal, fun-seeking, surrogate-parent, reservoir of family wisdom and distant styles Of grandparenting (based on those described by Neugarten and Weinstein, 1964); the personal and social dimensions of grandparental role meaning (items constructed by Robertson, 1977). Assessment Of the importance of daily involvement with the grandchild was based on questions devised by Kornhaber and Woodward (1981). These items were presented in a multiple- choice format in which the subject indicated degree of agreement or disagreement on a four-point scale. Original questions were develOped based on anecdotal evidence in the literature and from interviews with grandparents and expectant grandparents about intergenerational relationships (see Appendix C for keys to prenatal and postnatal questionnaires). These Open-ended questions were designed to elicit information to be used in generating future hypotheses and in revising the questionnaire for future studies. This section also was intended to provide information that might be salient at the time Of the transition to grandparenthood but which might not have been expressed by participants in previous studies who were already accustomed to the grandparental role. 53 Data Coding Methods Of coding the fixed response data were decided upon before the questionnaires were printed and computer coding information was then placed on each questionnaire. Changes were made when the questionnaires were returned, however, due to the manner in which many subjects answered questions 40 to 51 which requested age norms for various roles. Instead Of giving a particular age many subjects listed an age range. In part, this may have been due to the presence Of the computer column coding beside each question. For example, Question 40 asked the subject to fill in the best age to become a father and near the question were the numbers "(52-53)" indicating the columns this question referred to on the computer. Subjects may have used this information as an indication that a range was to be given. Therefore, the computer codebOOk was revised to allow 4 columns for each Of these questions replacing the coding on the questionnaires. The revised computer codebooks are found in Appendix D. These codebooks also provide a key showing how each fixed response was coded for each itenh and the method used for coding missing data. Babbie (1973) suggested that when the researcher is not sure Of the variety Of responses that an Open ended question will produce the coding scheme can be devised after the questionnaire has been given. This is done by preparing a list of the first responses, reviewing the 54 list, and then deciding on the kinds Of categories that are appropriate based not only on the aims of the questionnaire but on the nature of the responses themselves. Therefore, in order to categorize the Open ended data all the responses to an individual item were transcribed so that patterns in responses would be easier tO detect. In categorizing the prenatal data two experimenters developed a classification system independently and then the systems were combined and duplications eliminated. Two raters then categorized each response according to the classification system. The statistic, kappa, was used to assess the reliability. Those categories that led to lower reliabilities were redefined for each question and a second classification system was develOped. Three raters then categorized each response for every question and three kappa statistics were determined. Those questions for which the mean reliability between the raters remained below .7 after the revision were not categorized. In categorizing the postnatal data the investigator formed a classification system and 3 raters categorized the responses of the first 98 postnatal questionnaires that were returned. The classification Of questions for which the mean reliability was below .7 was then redone in an attempt to reduce the ambiguity between categories. Reliabilities between 3 raters were once again obtained and 55 only those above .7 were categorized. The prenatal and postnatal reliabilities are located in Appendix E. Any Open-ended questions that did not lead to a mutually exclusive category system were summarized. Prenatal data were coded from the questionnaires onto coding sheets by the eXperimenter and then professionally key punched onto cards. Random spot checks revealed no errors. Postnatal data were coded on the questionnaires by the investigator and transcribed onto coding sheets by an undergraduate research assistant. It was then entered into the computer interactively by two research assistants. Random spot checks Of 20% of the subjects revealed approximately one error per subject. Therefore, the responses of every postnatal questionnaire were compared to the responses on the computer and corrections were made. Analyses The chi square statistic was chosen as the apprOpriate statistical test for the Open-ended questions because the variables were categorical (Runyon and Haber, 1972). Since the chi square statistic requires a mutually exclusive category system (Hays, 1973, page 719), the classification scheme used for coding the Open-ended questions allowed a subject's response to fall into one and only one category. A second assumption for apprOpriate usecn?the chi square test is that "the outcomes for the N respective Observations in the sample are independent" (Hays, 1973). 56 Therefore, chi square analyses were performed only in those situations in which each response could be categorized independent of the other responses (e45, this statistic was not used in repeated measures involving the same subject so that only one entry appeared per subject in the joint frequency table). The final assumption, the requirement that the number of subjects in the sample be large” has been called, "the stickiest question of all.u" lJlChi square tests for association. Since statisticians do not agree on what constitutes a large enough sample size the conservative position advocatedtnrHays was used as a standard in this study: "".we will simply state a rule that is at least current, fairly widely endorsed, and generally conservative. For tables with more than a single degree of freedom, earninimum expected frequency CM‘S can be regarded as adequate, although when there is only a single degree Of freedom a minimum expected frequency Of‘Hlis much safer. This rule-Of-thumb is ordinarily conservative, however, and circumstances may arise where smaller expected frequencies can be tolerated. In particular, if the number of degrees of freedom is large, then it is fairly safe to use the chi square test for association even if the minimum expected frequency is as small as 1, provided that there are only a few cells with small eXpected frequencies (such as one out of five or fewer)" (Hays, 1973, page 736). By following this standard, the chi square analyses reported in this study were based only on the major classifications for each question; the most frequent result was the elimination of the "Other" category in these analyses. Table 1 illustrates why this was necessary. It can be seen that expected frequencies are adequate except 57 in the final category, "Other person". Although these low frequency categories are informative in a descriptive sense their inclusion in a statistical test of significance would result in a high level Of significance that would not reflect a real difference. pool per c categories in order to increase the expected frequency ell this procedure was not followed since: "The whole rationale for the chi square approximation rests on the randomness Of the sample, and that the categories into which observations may fall are chosen in advance. When one starts pooling the categories after the data are seen, he is doing something to the randomness of the sample, with unknown consequences for his inferences. The manner in which categories are pooled can have an important Although it is not unusual to effect on the inferences one draws" (Hays, 1973, page 736). Table 1: Joint Frequency Table. frequencies per cell. that you would be a grandparent? Illustration of expected Question: Who told you Total Category Label Male Female Father-tO-be 13 (11) 12 (14) 25 Mother-tO-be 23 (27) 37 (33) 60 Both eXpectant parents 13 (15) 20 (18) 33 Spouse 9 (5) 1 (6) 10 Other person 1 (1) 1 (1) 2 59 71 130 Note: EXpected frequency, reported in parentheses is determined by multiplying column frequency times row frequency divided by the total number of subjects. Full Sample. 58 In those analyses in which there was only 1 degree of freedom Yates' corrected chi square was used. Hays (1973, pages 724-725) points out that this correction improves the Pearson chi square test. In this correction "the absolute value of the difference between Observed and expected frequencies is reduced by Jifkn'each category before the squaring is carried out" (Hays, 1973, page 725). For example, in an analysis to determine whether there were sex differences in regard to whether the expectant parents had discussed the grandchild's name with the grandparent, the raw chi square = 9.42. After the Yates' correction, the corrected chi square = 8.34. Multivariate analysis of variance techniques were used to examine the forced-choice questions. Cluster analysis (PACKAGE program developed by John Hunter) reduced the number Of dependent variables from 31 tOEL In order to determine a cluster score each item was weighted according to its loading (”1 the cluster. For example, ES items contributed to the "Advisor" cluster. Their loadings on the cluster were: Q22: 61; Q19: 59; Q9: 50; Q26: 44; 02: 38. The sum of the loadingsr=0 (Emotional Satisfaction). These clusters were used as dependent variables in the multivariate analyses reported later in the paper. The postnatal correlation matrix, formed with the above 9 clusters, was replicated within sampling error limits. Table 7 shows intercorrelations between clusters found postnatally. By averaging the prenatal and postnatal intercorrelations sampling error is reduced. ‘Therefore, Table 7 also shows an averaged matrix. This matrix, which is less susceptible to error, can be used in order to generate hypotheses concerning causal relationships between these variables in order to generate a path model for testing in future research. However, it should be noted that in the Average matrix areas of major discrepancy 75 between the prenatal and postnatal matrices are not evident but Should be considered in any path model which eXplains the significance of the grandparental role. In order to compare subjects on prenatal and postnatal cluster scores a matched-pairs t—test was performed on each cluster (Longitudinal Selected Sample). In order to maintain a .05 significance level for the overall analysis the significance level for the individual comparisons was set to .005. None of the 9 comparisons was significant at this level: Involved, t(116) = .75; Surrogate Parent, t(116) = 1.58; Advisor, t(117) = .82; Distant Figure, t(117) = -1.55; Biological Renewal, t(116) = -1.52; Youthfulness, t(115) = -.59; Funseeker, t(117) = —.65; Emotional Satisfaction, t(117) = -n43; Vicarious Achievement, t(116) = 2.67. Therefore, subjects were consistent prenatally and postnatally in their assessments Of' the meaning cM‘ the grandparental role. 76 Table 7: Prenatal Correlation Matrix Involved SurrogateParent Advisor Distant Figure Bio. Ren. Youthfulness Funseeker Emo. Sat. Vic. Ach. Postnatal Correlation Matrix Involved SurrogateParent Advisor Distant Figure Bio. Ren. Youthfulness Funseeker Emo. Sat. Vic. Ach. Averaged Correlation Matrix** Involved SurrogateParent Advisor Distant Figure Bio. Ren. Youthfulness Funseeker Emo. Sat. Vic. Ach. *1 item cluster (alpha equals 1.00 by definition) .54 .60 .63 .65 .78 .50 .43 .58 .61 .57 .67 .66 .77 .62 .32 .52 x .57 .58 .65 .65 .78 .56 038 .55 I 32 3O -5 15 45 40 11 36 I 27 21 -22 -7 17 76 56 —1 I 30 26 -13 u 31 58 33 18 SP 22 34 27 47 15 41 SP 43 11 31 -19 73 40 SP 32 23 29 60 -2 58 41 -12 13 -6 12 27 -1O 18 18 -5 8 2O -8 9 10 Intercorrelations between clusters DF 15 -13 -46 —15 30 DF 12 -1O -59 -1O 21 DF 14 -12 ~52 -13 26 BR 53 22 48 42 BR 46 33 29 BR 50 111 41 36 77 58 48 29 88 61 53 73 54 F VA 64 11 36 F VA 54 —1O 51 F VA 59 1 43 HAveraged correlations rounded toward larger sample size. 77 AGE DIFFERENCES In order to determine whether there are age differences in expectations for the grandparental role it had been proposed that subjects be classified according to three groups: those 45-yearS-Old and younger, those 46 to 55 years of age, and those 56-yearS-Old and Older . 'This categorization, however, led to difficulties since cell Sizes in the youngest group were too small for statistical analysis (see Table 8). Therefore, analyses Of age differences are based upon 2 age groups rather than three. Table 8: Subject distribution by sex and age‘ Age Iales Females 45 years and younger 6 6 46 through 55 years 26 36 56 years and Older 27 27 52 years and younger 26 34 53 years and Older 33 35 *Table 8 is based on (available) selected sample distribution for MANOVA Of cluster by sex and age. 4 subjects are not included due to missing data. 78 Multivariate tests of significance analyzing each Of the clusters by sex and age revealed that there were no significant age differences in either the prenatal data, F(9,116) = 1.05, or the postnatal data, F(9,105) = .93. MANOVA also revealed no significant age differences when the 2-choice fixed response questions (not included in the clusters) assessing eXpectations for role behaviors were analyzed by sex and age, F(6,118) = 1.57. There were age differences in the two remaining fixed response questions. A 2 X 2 ANOVA (sex by age level) revealed that there were significant age differences in terms of whether persons were becoming grandparents at the expected age, F(1,122) = 28.4, p < .01, and whether they perceived themselves as young, middle-aged, or Old, F(1,127) = 17.8, p < .01. (There were no sex differences on either question). Figure 1 illustrates the age differences. As one would predict, subjects in the younger age group (52 years and under) reported on the average that they were becoming grandparents at about the time they had expected. Those in the Older group (53 and older) were becoming grandparents (on the average) a little later than expected. These findings were consistent in the postnatal data. Once again there were no sex differences but the age difference was significant, F(1,110) = 21.26, p < .01. 79 Figure 2 Shows the postnatal means for timing of grandparenthood. Comparison of Figures 1 and 2 shows that the means essentially remained the same. Prenatal- postnatal comparisons for each subject revealed no changes in perception Of timing, t(109) = - 1.1. In order to assess subjects' self-perceived age status they were asked to indicate whether they considered themselves to be young, middle-aged, or Old. However, in coding,these responses it was determined thatzaS-choice category scale would have to be used since some subjects checked between two of the boxes, as shown in Table 9. AS can be seen in Figure 3, those in the younger age group tended to see themselves as somewhere between being young and being middle-aged. Those in the Older group tended to see themselves as having reached middle age. The postnatal data, shown in Table 10, reveals that more subjects remained within the choices as Offered on the questionnaire. This would be an interesting tOpic to eXplore in future research: Was it the pending event Of grandparenthood that led several subjects to consider themselves somewhere between youth and middle-age? Did this event help them "become" middle-aged in their own perceptions Of aging? A 2 X 2 ANOVA (sex by age level) revealed that those in the younger age group still 80 considered themselves to have a younger perceived age status than those in the Older group did postnatally, F(1,113) = 10.8, p < .01. However, it can be seen in Figure 4 that the means Of both groups increased. In order to determine if this prenatal-postnatal change was significant a t-test for matched pairs was analyzed. It revealed that there was a significant increase in the means such that the postnatal scores reflected an Older age status than did prenatal scores, t(115) = - 3.08, p'<.01. 81 Later than I had eXpected (3) At about the time I had expected (2) 3': 1.98 Sooner than I had eXpected (1) 3? = 2.51 Figure 1: 52 years 53 years and and younger Older (n = 63) (n = 63) Prenatal EXpectations for Timing Of Grandparenthood Cell means for Question 32 When I think about the age I expected to be when I became a grandparent I realize that I will become one: (Selected Sample - 6 missing Observations) 82 Later than I had expected (3) x = 2.48 At about the time I had expected (2) 'Y 1.96 Sooner than I had eXpected (1) 52 years 53 years and and younger older (n=51) (n=63) Figure 2: Postnatal Perceptions for Timing Of Grandparenthood Cell means for Question 32 When I think about the age I expected tO be when I became a grandparent I realize that I became one: (Longitudinal Selected Sample: 3 missing observations) 83 Old (5) Between Middle Aged and Old (4) Middle Aged (3) x = 2.91 x = 2.22 Between Young and Middle Aged (2) Young (1) 52 years 53 years and and younger older (n :63) (n = 68) Figure 3: Prenatal Perceived Age Status Cell means for question 33 I consider myself to be: (Selected Sample - 1 missing observation) 84 Old (5) Between Middle Aged and Old (4) 7 = 3.08 Middle Aged (3) T = 2.62 Between Young and Middle Aged (2) Young (1) 52 years 53 years and and younger older (n =52) (n = 65) Figure 4: Postnatal Perceived Age Status Cell means for question 33 I consider myself to be: (Longitudinal Selected Sample) 85 Tablee9: Prenatal Frequencies for Perceived Age Status (Question 33) Category Label: Code Frequency I consider myself to be Young 1 20.6% Between Young and Middle Aged 2 12.2% Middle Aged 3 61.1% Between Middle Aged and Old 4 .8% Old 5 5.3% (based on N = 131) Table 10: Postnatal Frequencies for Perceived Age Status (Question 33) Category Label: Code Frequency I consider myself to be Young 1 11% Between Young and Middle Aged 2 0% Middle Aged 3 83.3% Between Middle Aged and Old 4 .9% Old 5 4.3% (based on N :11?) 86 Expected timing of grandparenthood The selected sample was used to determine whether there were differences between subjects tun: reported different perceptions in terms Of the timing Of grandparenthood. Although most of the subjects indicated that they were becoming grandparents at the anticipated age (49% - men; 58% women), others indicated that it was occurring sooner (13% - men; 6% - women) or later (31% - men; 34% - women) than expected. There was considerable variation in the chronological ages within each Of the groups. Those becoming grandparents sooner than expected ranged in age from 42- to 52-years-Old (R = 47.4); those becoming grandparents at the eXpected age ranged from 37- to 77-years-old (;==52.5);those becoming grandparents later than expected ranged from 45 to 68 years of age (it = 56.8). Those reporting that they were becoming grandparents earlier than anticipated tended to have younger children (age of youngest child ranged from 12 to 22 years, i’: 17.75) than those at the anticipated age (range: 12 years - 33 years; R = 20.56) and those who were becoming grandparents later than expected (range: 13 to 36 years; I: 25.86). Since there was rm) relationship between sex and perceived timing Of grandparenthood, {H2,N=126) = 2.49, p = .287, both sexes were combined in analyses to determine if the timing of grandparenthood was related to the person's anticipated interactions with the grandchild. The majority of subjects reported expectations for frequent interactions 87 with the grandchild, indicating comfort in the role regardless of timing. FOr example, subjects anticipated babysitting (79% - men; 93% women), engaging in home recreational activities with the grandchild (90% - men; 100% - women), relating family history (67% - men; 90% women) and teaching skills to the grandchild (59% - men; 80% - women). An association was found between persons' perceptions Of when they expected to become grandparents and whether they had p1ans to babysit the grandchild, x2(2,N=126) = 7.69, p < .05, such that those who were becoming grandparents at the expected time were more likely to have these plans. There was also a relationship between perceived timing and plans to teach the grandchild special Skills, x42,N=124) =9.42, p < .01. Only 33% Of those becoming grandparents sooner than expected planned to teach various skills whereas most (77% Of those becoming grandparents at the expected time, 72% Of those becoming grandparents later than expected) of the other grandparents had these plans. No differences were found between perceived timing and plans to relate family history, x2(2,N=124) = 1.8, or plans to engage in home recreational activities, {H2,N=126) = .57. It had been hypothesized that there would be a relationship between the perceived timing of grandparenthood and persons' reports Of difficulty thinking of themselves as grandparents. This hypothesis approached the preset level of significance, x2(6,N=125) = 12.0, p = .06. The fact that 88 many subjects (33% - men; 14% - women) found it difficult to think Of themselves as grandparents also may be due to the fact that the grandchildren had not yet been born. However, even after the birth Of the grandchild 25% Of the grandfathers and 15% Of the grandmothers had difficulty thinking Of themselves in this role. Ideal age to become a grandparent Neugarten, Moore and Lowe (1965) reported that major life events occur in a prescriptive timetable and that events in most persons lives follow this timetable. They found that a middle-aged, middle-class sample reported that the best age to become grandparents would be between 45 and 50 years of age. In the current study this issue was addressed to determine what expectant grandparents consider to be the best age to become a grandparent. Comparisons were then made with the person's actual chronological age to determine how many were becoming grandparents at what they considered to be the ideal age. (These analyses are based only on those respondents who gave an exact age or age range, eg. 45 tO 60 years, rather than a range with no end point, eg. 45 years and up. When a range was given the average of the range was used in the computations reported below). When asked the best age to become a grandfather the responses Of the expectant grandparents ranged from 40- years-Old to 62-years-Old with a mean age Of 50.3 years (n 89 = 106). The modal reSponse, 50-years-Old, was given by 30.2% Of the subjects. When men's and women's responses were analyzed separately they were found to be similar: men reported a mean best age as 50.0 years (n = 50) and women reported a mean best age Of 5057 years (n = 56). When asked the best age for a woman to become a grandmother the responses from both sexes ranged from 37- years-old to 60-years—Old with a mean of 49.2 years. Once again the modal response was 50 years Of age, suggested by 25.5% of the subjects. When analyzed separately, men (n = 50) reported a best age oleL9 years for women to become grandmothers; women reported a best age of 49.4 years (n = 56). Similar results were found in the postnatal data, however, the upper limits Of the ranges were extended. The grandparents reported that the best age to become a grandfather ranged from 40- to 75-years—Old with a mean age Of 50.4 years (n=98). The modal response, which remained at 50-yearS-Old, was given by 31.6% Of the subjects. When responses were analyzed by sex both men and women reported a mean best age Of 50.4 years (men's responses ranged from 40 years tO 75 years; women's responses ranged from 40 years to 65 years). Postnatally, subjects indicated that the best age to become a grandmother ranged from 40 to 69 years of age with a mean age Of 49J)years. The modal response, 50 years of age, was given by 20.4% of the subjects. Men's responses 90 (n=45) ranged from 40- to 69-years-Old with a mean age of 48.8 years. Women's responses (n=53) ranged from 40 to 65 years of age with a mean age Of 49.2 years. Each subject's ideal age was compared to his or her chronological age (thus creating an age differential variable) resulting in three groups: (1) those becoming a grandparent before the best age to become one (chronological age less than best age); (2) those becoming grandparents at the best age to become one (chronological age equals best age); (3) those becoming grandparents after the best age to become one (chronological age greater than best age). Table 11 shows the frequencies for each group. There were I“) sex differences ix) comparisons Of chronological age to best age at first grandparenthood, x2(2, N = 105) = .845. A comparison Of the age differential for subjects with their responses for expected timing led to a significant relationship, X2 (4, N = 102) = 26.03, p < .01. Table 12 illustrates this relationship. (Note: These analyses could not be performed in the postnatal data due to low expected frequencies; only 3 subjects were becoming grandparents at the best age which leads to extremely small eXpected frequency cell SizesJ Of those subjects tn“) were becoming grandparents before what they considered to be the best age, 26.9% were becoming grandparents sooner than expected and 73.1% were becoming grandparents at about the time they had expected. 91 76.9% of those whose chronological age was equal to their best age for first grandparenthood reported that they were becoming grandparents at about the time they had eXpected, whereas 15.4% were becoming grandparents later than expected and 7.7% were becoming grandparents sooner than expected. Finally, among those subjects who were older than what they considered to be the best age to become a grandparent, 47.6% were becoming grandparents later than expected and 47.6% were becoming grandparents at the expected time with only 4.8% becoming grandparents sooner than expected. These differences are provocative in that they indicate that the persons involved in the study are not relying only on their own chronological age or what they see as an ideal age to become a grandparent when they determine their eXpected age at first grandparenthood and decide whether this pregnancy was sooner, at the expected time, or later than expected. An Obvious area for follow-up in future studies would be to determine whether certain aspects Of their child's develOpment (eg, chronological age, number of years married, assessment of maturity, completion Of education, career plans, etc.) lead them to a particular expected age for first grandparenthood. The particular norms followed by individuals of different cultural backgrounds also would be interesting to explore. 92 Table 11: Age Differential: Comparison of Chronological Age and Best Age to Become Grandparent Category Label N Freq. Becoming Grandparent Before Best Age 26 24.8% Becoming Grandparent at the Best Age 13 12.4% Becoming Grandparent After Best Age 66 62.9% (based on n = 105) Selected Sample Table 12: Q32 (EXpected Age for Grandparenthood) by Age Differential Before At After Best Best Best Age Age Age Sooner than n = 7 n = 1 n = 3 expected 63.6% 9.1% 27.3% 26.9% 7.7% 4.8% At expected n = 19 n = 10 n = 30 time 32.2% 16.9% 50.8% 73.1% 76.9% 47.6% Later than n = 0 n = 2 n = 30 eXpected 0% 6.3% 93.8% 0% 15.4% 47.6% (based on n = 102) First percentage in each cell represents the row percentage; second percentage in each cell represents the column percentage. Selected Sample. 93 LINEAGE Anticipating Grandparenthood It was not expected that there would be differences in when maternal and paternal grandparents began to imagine themselves in the grandparental role. One reason for this expectation was due to the fact that most of the subjects had other children besides the eXpectant parent and so they may have imagined themselves as maternal and paternal grandparents. (In fact, in future research it would be interesting to ask each subject, "Is this the child that you eXpected to have your first grandchild" and follow up on the responses given). Chi square tests of Significance did not reveal an association between lineage and when the person imagined the grandparental role, X2(7,N=118)=5.51. When they learned that they were to become grandparents both maternal and paternal grandparents most Often reported a positive reaction (88.2% of maternal; 77.2% of paternal). Information about the pregnancy A 2 X 4 Chi Square analysis of lineage by who-told- the—subject- Of- the- pregnancy was performed to determine if grandparents were informed of the pregnancy by their own children. A significant association 1was found, x2(3,N=127)=51.6, p 1(.01. Table 13 shows the pattern Of responses. It is interesting to note that none of the 94 maternal grandparents became aware of the pregnancy through their sons-in-law. As would be expected, most learned from their daughters. Although paternal grandparents were most Often told Of the pregnancy by their sons, 17.9% received the news from their daughters-in-law. It seems that news of pregnancy is considered either the adult child's news or the expectant mother's news. Although it had been anticipated that maternal grandparents (especially the grandmother) may have been more aware Of whether or not a pregnancy was being planned this was not the case. There was no association between lineage and whether the children had let the parents know a pregnancy was being planned, x2(3,N=122)=5.02, or between lineage and how the parents became aware that their children planned to have a baby, x2(3,N=54)=.97. Table 13: Lineage differences regarding the person by whom the subject was told of the pregnancy Lineage Informed Of Pregnancy by: Maternal Paternal Father to be n = 0 n = 25 0% 44.6% Mother to be n = 49 n = 10 69% 17.9% Both expectant parents n = 15 n = 18 21.1% 32.1% Spouse n = 7 n = 3 9.9% 5.4% % = column percentages 95 Information about the birth There was a significant association in terms of how maternal grandparents compared 1vith paternal grandparents learned that the grandchild had been born, x2(1,N=97) = 10.05, p < .01. Table 14 reveals the nature Of this association. It can be seen that the maternal grandparents learned the news attfluehospital more frequently than did the paternal grandparents. When asked if they were at the hospital during the birth, maternal grandparents were more likely to report that they were, x2(1,N=115)==7386, p < .01. Expected frequencies were too low in several cells for statistical analyses of lineage in terms of who told the grandparent that the grandchild had been born. 'Table 15, however, Shows interesting patterns to pursue in future research. For example, only maternal grandparents actually saw or heard their grandchildren being born (of these 4 were maternal grandmothers and ‘1 was :3 maternal grandfather). Although information about the pregnancy was most Often relayed by the person's child or by the eXpectant mother, information about the birth is most Often conveyed tx> maternal and paternal grandparents by the new father. Since all but 7 subjects had seen their grandchild within the first month, statistical analyses for lineage differences in terms Of the age Of the grandchild when 96 first seen were performed using only the first three time periods (see Table 5 for comparison). There was a significant association between these variables, x2(2,N=110) = 14.27, p < .01. As seen in Table 16, maternal grandparents tended to see the grandchild earlier than did paternal grandparents. Maternal and paternal grandparents were compared to determine if there was a difference in how Often they saw their grandchildren. NO association was found, x2(5,N=100) = 4.82, when the categories "Daily" to "Quarterly" visits were compared (see Table 6 for detail of categories). Since this analysis involved cells with low expected frequencies and since 76% of the subjects reported seeing their grandchildren at least once 23 month, an analysis was performed to determine if there was a difference among the subjects seeing their grandchildren frequently. Again, no association 'was found, x?(3,N=89)=6.9. There were also no differences in how frequently maternal and paternal grandparents expected to see their grandchildren in the first year, X2(6,N=96>=11.18. 97 Table 14: Lineage differences in learning of the grandchild's birth Lineage Grandparent learned of birth Maternal Paternal n=23 n:4 At hospital for birth 41.1% 9.8% Through phone call n = 33 n = 37 58.9% 90.2% Longitudinal Selected Sample. N = 97. Categories "Learned in person after birth" and "Other" could not be included due to low expected frequencies per cell. Column percentages. 98 Table 15: Lineage differences in who told the grandparents of the grandchild's birth Lineage Category Label Maternal Paternal New mother n = 6 n = 1 New father n = 35 n = 43 44.9% 55.1% Both new parents n = 2 n = 0 100% 0.0% My Spouse n = 9 n = 5 64.3% 35 7% Other person n = 3 n = 1 75.0% 25.0% Saw or heard birth n = 5 n = 0 100% 0 0% (Longitudinal Selected Sample) Table 16: Lineage differences in age of grandchild when first seen by grandparent Lineage Age Seen Maternal Paternal 1 day old or less n = 49 n = 23 68.1% 31.9% First week n = 10 n = 12 45.5% 54.5% First month n = 3 n = 13 18.8% 81.3% (Longitudinal Selected Sample) 99 Names There were no associations between lineage and whether the expectant parents had discussed names for the grandchild, {3(1,N=127)==0, whether the grandparents had suggested names, X2(2,N=127) = 1.2, whether the grandparents had suggested that the baby be named after anyone, X2(1,N=125) = 1.13, or whether the eXpectant grandparents own parents or in-laws had suggested names, x2(1,N=124) = .009. There were also no associations between lineage and reactions to the name given to the grandchild, x2 (2,N:105) = .60. Sex Preferences Only 25 subjects expressed a preference for a particular sexed grandchild before the birth. Of these, V? were maternal grandparents and (3 were paternal grandparents. Although cell Sizes are too small for statistical analysis, Table 17 shows the response pattern. NO association was found between sex preference and lineage in the postnatal data, x2(1,N=55) =.75. Table 17: Lineage and sex preferences Maternal Paternal GF GM GF GM Preference for Girl n=2 n=10 n=1 n=2 Preference for Boy n=3 n=2 n=5 n:0 (GF 2 Grandfather; GM = Grandmother) Selected Sample. 100 Plans to Help During the Neonatal Period Hypothesis 3 stated that anecdotal evidence suggests that help patterns during the neonatal period are influenced by lineage. In particular, the maternal grandparents may consider themselves to have priority over the paternal grandparents in terms Of aiding the new parents. An association was found between lineage and whether the grandparents planned to help the new parents around the time Of birth, x2(2, N=124)=8.24, p < .05. Table 18 reveals a pattern Of responses in which maternal grandparents were more likely to indicate that they were planning to help - 79.4% Of the expectant mothers' parents had these plans as compared to 55.4% Of the eXpectant fathers' parents. Paternal grandparents were more likely to reSpond that they would be helping out if needed or if an emergency arose than were maternal grandparents (paternal grandparents, EML1%; maternal grandparents, ‘HL7%). Subjects also were asked whether their own plans depended upon their childrens' in-laws plans. Once again, a relationship was found when this variable was examined by lineage, x2(2,N=121)=11.87, p < .01. In Table 19, it can be seen that more paternal grandparents than maternal grandparents indicated that their plans are tied to the plans of the other set of grandparents. 101 Table 18: Lineage difference in plans to help out during the neonatal period Maternal Paternal Grandparents Grandparents Yes, planning to help n : 54 n = 31 79.4% 55.4% NO, not planning to help n = 4 n = 7 5.9% 12.5% Planning to help if n = 10 n = 18 needed or if an 14.7% 32.1% emergency arises (% = column percentages) Table 19: Lineage differences in whether plans to help out the expectant parents are dependent on the other set Of grandparents' plans Maternal Paternal Grandparents Grandparents Yes, subject's plans n = 7 n = 19 n=26 depend on child's 10.3% 35.8% in-law's plans NO, subject's plans n = 59 n = 32 n:91 do not depend on 86.8% 60.4% child's in-law's plans Perhaps, subject's n = 2 n = 2 n:4 plans may depend 2.9% 3.8% on child's in-law's plans n=68 0:53 (% = column percent) 102 Help Patterns in the Neonatal Period Given the expectations Of more maternal grandparents that they would help out after the birth an analysis Of lineage by actual helping behavior during the neonatal period was performed. Contrary to eXpectations, no significant association was found, X2 (1,N=111) = .15. Biological Renewal Hypothesis 4 predicted that the meaning of grandparenthood in terms Of biological renewal may be influenced by lineage. TWun;is, there may be differences between those becoming grandparents through sons and those becoming grandparents through daughters. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that this lineage effect would occur among same-sexed parents and adult children. Maternal and paternal grandparents were compared to determine if they held different Opinions on feelings of biological renewal in regard to the birth Of the grandchild. Biological renewal scores were computed by averaging a subject's responses on questions pertaining to the importance Of the grandchild in that he or she will be carrying on the family line and the importance Of seeing the blood line continued for another generation. A 2 X 2 analysis of variance (sex by lineage) indicated the possibility of an interaction between sex and lineage F(1,123)= 3.66, p =.058. Figure 5 illustrates this interaction. 11: can be seen that maternal grandmothers and paternal grandfathers have lower 103 mean scores indicating stronger feelings Of biological renewal. In order to investigate this issue further an analysis of the postnatal data (2 X 2 ANOVA) for biological renewal and maternal/paternal lineage was performed. The interaction was not significant, F(1,110) = 1.61. This could have been due to the fact that the same- sexed lineage pattern was not continued in the next generation (example: a woman's daughter gave birth to a son thereby reducing feelings of biological renewal). Therefore, to further eXplore this issue postnatal scores on biological renewal were analyzed by 3- generational lineage patterns. Figure 6 illustrates the sex patterns found once the grandchildren had been born. As can be seen, cell sizes were too small 1H) permit statistical analysis Of each Of the 8 possible lineage combinations. Therefore, in order to determine whether stronger indications cfi‘ biological renewal would be Obtained for same-sexed lineage patterns Groups 1 and 8 were combined (Group 1: Grandparent-Male; Parent-Male; Grandchild-Male; Group 8: Grandparent-Female; Parent- Female; Grandchild-Female) and compared in) the remaining six groups. It was hypothesized that the mean in the same- sexed lineage patterns would be lower (indicating higher biological renewal) than the mean for the other patterns. Statistical analysis, however, revealed no significant differences, t(112) = -1.17. 104 Strongly Agree 1.0 1.5 2.0 O (2.39) (2.43) x 2.5 (2.56) O x (2.77) 300 3.5 x Grandmothers Strongly 4.0 O Grandfathers Disagree Maternal Paternal Figure 5: Interaction between sex and lineage on feelings of biological renewal. Selected Sample. Male Female n: 51 =64 Male emale Male Female n: 24 n=26 n=38 /\ /\ /\ /\ Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female n: 11 n: 13 n: 8 n: 19 n=13 n=13 n=15 n=23 Gr.1 Gr.2 Gr.3 Gr.4 Gr.5 Gr.6 Gr.7 Gr.8 Figure 6: Sex patterns in lineage. Longitudinal Selected Sample. 2 subjects missing - becoming maternal and paternal grandparents at the same time. 105 SEX DIFFERENCES Multivariate analysis Of variance techniques (Clusters x Sex x Lineage) were performed1on the Selected Sample in order to investigate whether there were sex or lineage differences ikn‘ each of the E) dependent variables identified through the cluster analysis (selected sample used for analysis). Since the interaction effect (Sex x Lineage) was not significant, F(9,13) = .95, the main effects were explored. Lineage also was not Significant, F(9,113) = 1.26, however there was a significant sex difference, F(9,113)==3.63,;>< .001. Univariate F-tests revealed that ‘two clusters showed ea significant sex difference: Involved, F(1,121) = 11.3, p < .001; Distant, F(1,121) = 15.2, p < .001. Comparisons of the means of each sex Show that women tend to score higher in terms of involvement with the grandchild and men tend to score higher on the Distant cluster. A multivariate analysis of variance on the Longitudinal Selected Sample did not confirm the above findings in the postnatal data. Once again the interaction was not significant, FT9,103) = .76, but when the main effects were explored it was found that lineage and sex also were not significant, F(9,103)==.66 for lineage and F(9,103) = 1.68 for sex. 106 Anticipating Grandparenthood Since it was hypothesized that there may be sex differences in regard to variables addressed in the Open ended questions, chi square tests Of significance were used to assess associations between sex and other qualitative variables. Men and women differed in regard to when they first began to imagine themselves as grandparents, x47,N=119) = 19.7, p < .01. Table 20 shows these responses. It can be seen that women report imagining themselves as grandparents earlier than (k) men. For example, almost twice as many of the women (48.5% to 23.6%) reported that they began to imagine themselves in this role either when they were children, when they were raising their children, or when their children began to get married. 29.1% Of the men but only 4.7% Of the women either had not yet begun to imagine themselves as grandparents or had only begun to do so after they had agreed to participate in this project and had started filling out this questionnaire. When asked to describe their first reactions to the news Of impending grandparenthood most subjects reported a positive reaction (79.3% Of the men; 83.1% Of the women). It is interesting tO note that those who did not respond in a positive manner had varying reactions that differed by sex. For example, 6.9% of the men had neutral reactions whereas nonecn?the women did. Seven percent of the women had mixed reactions, but no men did. 107 Table 20: When I was a child When I was raising my children When my children started to get married During the pregnancy itself When I was peers becoming grandparents When I reached a particular age When I began filling out this questionnaire Haven't begun to imagine it yet Male n 36. 108 UL) U'1 LA)" 20 4% 55 —-I 0 0‘1! 12’" Female 26 .6% 64 Sex differences ixl imagining oneself as a grandparent. 38 39 13 16 119 Information about the Pregnancy There were no sex differences regarding the knowledge of whether or not the pregnancy was planned, X2 (3,N=123) : 6.9, or in the cases of planned pregnancies, in how the parents became aware that their adult children were planning a pregnancy, x2(3,N=55):=‘L9. There were sex differences in regard to the person by whom they were told about the actual pregnancy, x2 (3,N=128) = 10.2, p < .05. Table 21 reveals the pattern of sex differences. It can be seen that a major difference lies in the number of men who are told the news by their spouses. Although nine men report that they learned that they were about to become grandfathers from their wives, only one woman reported learning the news from her husband. Furthermore, although she was told of the news by her spouse her remarks indicate that it was a family experience: "Our daughter and her husband came to visit and after they had been there for a while we decided to have some drinks. My husband said tO me - 'I don't know if I should mix a drink for a grandmother or notv It didn't register for a few seconds but then I realized what he meant. Our son-in-law had taken him aside and told him the good newsJ' (Subject #250) 109 Table 21: Sex differences: Who told the subjects that they would be grandparents Sex Category Label Male Female Father—tO-be n = 13 n = 12 25 22.4% 17.1% Mother-tO-be n = 23 n = 37 60 39.7% 52.9% Both expectant parents n = 13 n = 20 33 22.4% 28.6% Spouse n = 9 n = 1 10 15.5% 1.4% 58 70 128 (% = column percent) Information about the Birth There were no sex differences in terms of whether subjects were at the hospital during the birth, X2(1,N=117) = 2.67, nor were there differences among those who saw their grandchildren within the first month in regard to the age Of the grandchild when first seen, x2(2,N=110) = 3.0. Among those who reported continued visits with the grandchildrencniat least a monthly basis no association was found between sex and involvement, x2(3,N=89) = 1.23. 110 Names There were sex differences in regard to whether the expectant parents had discussed the grandchild's name with the grandparent, x2(1,N=128) = 8.34, p < .01. 73.9% of the women reported that they had discussed names with their children whereas only 47.5% Of the men stated that they had these discussions. Given this information it is not surprising, therefore, that there is also a sex difference in regard to whether the grandparent had suggested a name, x2(2,N=128) = 13.65, p < Ark 34.3% Of the women but only 10.3% Of the men had suggested a name. An additional 4.3% of the wonmn1(but no men) did not suggest a specific name but did make general suggestions (eg, recommended that a short first name be chosen due to a long last name). It is interesting to note that women were more likely than men to report that their own parents and in-laws had suggested names when they were expecting children, x2(1,N=125) = 4.66, p'<.05. Of the 17 subjects reporting that their parents had made suggestions 82.4% were women. However, it should be emphasized that the majority of both sexes did not report that their parents had made suggestions. When asked how they felt about the name that was eventually given to their grandchildren there were no sex differences, x2(2,N=107) = 1.07. 111 Sex Preferences As noted earlier the majority Of subjects did not eXpress a preferenceefor the sex Of their grandchild when questioned prenatally. Therefore, differences between grandfathers and grandmothers in their report Of a preferred sex were not statistically analyzed. Instead, they are presented in tabular form (see Table 22). Of those subjects who preferred a granddaughter, 80% were women and 20% were men; of those who preferred grandsons, 80% were men and 20% were women. A tendency to prefer same-sexed grandchildren could be assessed in future studies to determine if this is a reliable difference and if‘so, what reasons grandparents give for this preference (eg, having skills that would be Of particular interest to a same-sexed grandchild, etc.). Table 22: Subject's sex by preference for grandchild's sex Male Female Preference for Girl n = 3 n = 12 15 Preference for Boy n = 8 n = 2 10 11 14 25 Since more subjects did indicate that they had preferred a grandchild Of a particular sex in the postnatal responses, a chi square analysis was performed to see if there was an association between subject's sex and the preferred sexcfi‘the grandchild. The association was not 112 Significant, x2 (1,N=55) = .29. Howevery this issue should be eXplored in future studies before and after the birth of the first grandchild. It could be that the postnatal preference actually was not indicative of a preference beforerthe birth, especially since there was a significant association between a postnatal report that the subject had been hOping that the grandchild was Of a particular sex and having a grandchild Of that sex, x2(1,N=55) = 19.22, p < .01. Plans to Help During the Neonatal Period Chi square tests (Sex x Plans-to-help-out) revealed a Significant association between grandparents' sex and their intentions tx> help out at the time 10f birth, x2(2,N=125) = 10.49, p < .01. ‘Women more frequently report plans to help than do men (79.7% of women vs. 55.4% Of men). An additional 28.6% Of the women and 17.4% of the men reported that they would help if needed or if an emergency arose. When asked whether their plans depended on the plans Of the in-lawsq no sex difference was found, x2(2,N=122) = 4.35. There was a relationship between sex and whether plans to help had been discussed with the expectant parents, X2(2,N=122) = 6.96, p < .05. Table 23 shows that women were more likely to report discussing plans for helping out at the time of birth with their children. 113 Table 23: Subjectfls sex bythe hospital. Took both daughter-in-law and granddaughter home. Having previously visited then: whenever possible. Our independent daughter-in-law handled everything herself, and very well. We made suggestions when solicited, but that's all. Yes! Meals 8 Baby sitting Yes--in a way--I stayed at our own Home to care for things while my wife helped care for baby the 2nd wk mother 8 baby was home from the hospital. Some chores around the house Yes-Babysat for 2 hours Yes. Helped out ix: re-arranging the furniture at home - brought up by trailer additional furniture for the parents comfort. Went food shOpping. 119 Daily Involvement with the Grandchild Grandparents indicated that they had little interest in assuming a surrogate parent role. All the grandfathers and all but one grandmother agreed that parenting should be left strictly tO the parents. Eighty percent Of the grandfathers and 92% Of the grandmothers did not feel that grandparents have an Obligation to help care for the grandchildren on a regular basis. However, 98% Of the men and all of the women stated that they would help out in family emergencies. Men and women differed in regard to whether or not they felt grandparents and grandchildren should live near one another t(127) : 4.59, p < .001, and in their indication that there should be daily involvement with the grandchild, t(129) = 3.03, p < .05. Few grandfathers (16%) indicated that it was important for grandparents to live close to their grandchildren and fewer still (5%) felt that grandparents should be involved in the daily lives Of their grandchildren. However, 51% Of the women agreed that grandparents should live nearby with 14% stating that they should be involved in their grandchildren's daily lives. It is interesting to note that although there is a strong relationship between reporting that grandparents should live close to their grandchildren and reporting that grandparents should be involved in their grandchildren's daily lives, x2 (9,N=131) = 38.9, p < .001, in terms Of actual living styles these variables were not strongly 120 related to the geographical distance between grandchild and grandparent (importance of living close and actual geographical distance: x2(9,N=131) = 16.3, p =.06; need for daily involvement. and geographical distance: x2(9.N=130) = 3.3, p = .95. GRANDPARENTAL ROLE MEANING TYPES Replication of Robertson's Factor Analysis Robertson (1977) identified four grandparental role meaning types. These types were determined through factor analytic techniques. From a pool of 20 Likert-type items her analysis revealed that 12 were significant (p‘<.05). Table 25 (adapted from Robertson, 1977, page 168) shows the items (and factor loadings) used by Robertson to assess role meaning and the corresponding items from the present study. Table 26 lists those items used in the replication analysis. It can be seen that 3 items were not included (33, 38, nu). It was felt that these items (concerning advice on successful marriages, choosing occupations, and taking a pleasure trip around the world) were too far in the future to be meaningful to a sample that did not yet have grandchildren. Therefore the replication included A items for the Social Dimension and 5 items for the Personal Dimension. Minor changes were made in the wording of these items to make them applicable to an expectant grandparent. 121 Table 25: Role meaning items and factor loadings* McGreal Item # NA 20 25 23 28 NA 18 27 Robertson Item # 33 30 29 37 31 38 3M “3 in Item I would tell my grandchildren to always remember that love and companionship are more important to a successful marriage than money. Going to visit a friend for Christmas is more enjoyable than having Christmas with one's family. The greatest happiness is found in a family where all members work together as a group. I feel that I should do what is morally right to set a good example for my grandchildren. One of the most important things I want from my grandchildren is for them to "respect their elders". I feel that my grandchildren should be encouraged to choose their own occupation whether their parents agree or disagree with their choice. Life would be very lonely for for me without my grandchildren. The most important thing about having grandchildren is that they have brought a deep sense of emotional satisfaction to my life. One of the most important things about having grand- children for me is that they make me feel young again. 122 .701 .606 .562 .999 .fl56 .H26 .267 .222 .118 II .056 .090 .119 .186 .250 .113 .543 0526 .955 Table 25 (continued) McGreal Robertson Item I II Item # Item # NA uu If one of my grandchildren -.O75 .412 had the Opportunity to take a pleasure trip around the world, I think he should take the trip first and see life while he is still young and worry about going to college and getting a job later. 21 MO I think I should be able to -.002 .3u9 give my grandchildren whatever I can and not be worried about spoiling them. 16 26 One of the most important -.009 .393 things about having grand- children is that they provide me with a way to see my blood line carried on for another generation. *Adapted from Robertson, 1977, Table 1. I = Factor 1: Social Dimension Factor II = Factor II: Personal Dimension Factor 123 Since Robertson specified the use of a rotated factor matrix but didrufi:specify the type of rotation, the most widely used rotation method (Varimax) was chosen (Nie, Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner and Bent, 1975) and creation of 2 factors was specified. Table 26 shows the factor loadings for each variable. It can be seen that the variables loading on each factor are as predicted from Robertson's analysis in both the prenatal and the postnatal analyses: Q3, Q16, Q18, Q21, and Q27 loaded highest on the Personal Dimension; Q20, Q23, Q25, and Q28 loaded highest on the Social Dimension. Robertson reported that the Personal Dimension Factor and the Social Dimension Factor explained approximately 84% of the variance. In the prenatal replication they explained 41.4% of the total variance; in the postnatal replication they eXplained 40.9% of the total variance. The differences between Robertson's results and those found in the current investigation may be due, :h1 part, to differences in sample characteristics. Whereas Robertson's sample included subjects with considerable experience with the grandparental role, the replication sample needed to anticipate and speculate about the role they were about to assume when assessed prenatally and they remained relatively ineXperienced as grandparents in the postnatal analysis. Also, Robertson's sample consisted of women 124 whereas this sample included responses from both sexes. Finally, Robertson's analysis was based on 12 items whereas in the present study only 9 were used. The factor loadings presented in Table 26 are based on computations using a correlation matrix with unities in the diagonal (since this appears to be the definition of the factors in Robertson's analysis). Hunter and Gerbing (1982) point out that "the choice of the diagonal elements is not arbitrary, but determines the definition of the factors to be defined in the analysis". They note that the use of unities in the diagonal lead to "correlations between the items and the observed cluster score rather than its underlying trait. The correlation of an item with its own cluster score is the item-total correlation, and is greatly inflated by a spurious common error termJ' However, when communalities are placed in the diagonal then the "item-factor correlations are the estimated correlations between items and cluster true scores (eg, the underlying trait scoreSL Table 27 presents factor loadings based on a correlational matrix:with.communalitiescn1the diagonal. Comparison of Tables 26 and 27 reveals that the items on the personal dimension retain higher factor loadings than 125 do those items on the Social Dimension when communalities are used. The artificial inflation due to error scores on the Social Dimension may be due to the small number of items (4) assessing that trait. Therefore, although factor analysis confirms the two factors defined by Robertson, further research should include an additional battery of items which would be hypothesized to intercorrelate with the Social Dimension Factor. Table 26: Factor Loadings: Principle Components Solution Item # and Description 25. Family members work together 23. Set a good example 20. Visit friend for holidays 28. ReSpect for elders 27. Bring emotional satisfaction 18. Lonely without grandchildren 16. Blood line carried on 3. Make me feel young again 21. Give without worry of spoiling 126 Factor I Factor II (Social) (Personal) Pre Post Pre Post .703* .633* -.064 .207 .576* .718* .066 .031 -.501* -.641* .037 .067 .492* .583' .340 .210 .194 .048 .781* .757” .176 .103 .648' .613* -.139 .197 .643* .578* .139 —.121 .625* .724* -.196 .076 .550* .215* Table 27: Factor Loadings: Principle Factor Solution Factor I Factor II Item # and Description Pre Post Pre Post 25. Family members work together .683* .490* -.124 .193 23. Set a good example .235* .581* .091 .053 20. Visit friend for holidays -.176* .434* -.013 .006 28. Respect for elders .415* .433* .240 .187 27. Bring emotional satisfaction .250 .071 .763* .671* 18. Lonely without grandchildren .157 .123 .527* .455* 16. Blood line carried on -.018 .183 .509* .432* 3. Make me feel young again .171 -.053 .486* .579* 21. Give without worry of spoiling -.044 .068 .386* .127* Principle components solution places unities on the diagonals. Principle factor solution places communalities on the diagonals. * indicates the factor on which the item has the highest loading. 127 Role Meaning Types In order to categorize subjects according to typologies, subjects received a score of 1 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree) for each of the 5 personal dimension items and for each of the 4 social dimension items. (Subjects who did not answer all questions pertaining to a dimension were not included in the analyses reported below.) The possible range of scores on the personal dimension was from 5 (indicating strong agreement on all the questions) to 20 (indicating strong disagreement on all the questions). The actual range was from 61x>18 ()7 = 13) in the prenatal data and from 6 to 20 (x = 13) in the postnatal data. The possible range for the social dimension scores was 4 to 16. The actual range was from 5 to 13 (i = 9) in the prenatal data and from 4 to 12 (i = 9) in the postnatal data. A mean split was used on the prenatal data to dichotomize subjects into a high-agreement group and a low- agreement group on each dimension. Those subjects with a mean score were randomly assigned into a group (as assigned by Robertson, 1977). Sixty-nine subjects were classified as high scorers and 71 as low scorers on the personal dimension. The higher scoring group on the social dimension was composed of 67 subjects; the low scoring group had 72 subjects. The two dimensions were combined in order to categorize subjects according to Robertson's (1977) four grandparental role Ineaning groups. 128 Table 28 compares the distribution of grandmothers obtained by Robertson with the distribution of grandparents found in the current study. In both studies, the most common types of grandparental roles are the apportioned role type and the remote role type. Robertson (1977) found that these types varied on several life style variables. However, the significance levels used in her study were liberal. Therefore, traditional levels of significance were used in the current investigation in order to determine the usefulness of this typology. Table 28: Comparison of grandparental role meaning types. Personal Dimension High Low Apportioned Symbolic Robertson Robertson n=36 n=33 High McGreal McGreal n=38 n=28 Social Dimension Individualized Remote Robertson Robertson n=21 11:35 Low McGreal McGreal n = 29 n = 41 Robertson's data adapted from Robertson, 1977, Table 2. McGreal data based on Full Sample. There iwas no relationship between grandparental role type and sex, x2(3,N=136) = 3.41. Therefore, men's and women's scores were analyzed together in the procedures reported above. Although the association between lineage (maternal vs. paternal) and role types approached the preset level of significance, x7—(3, N=133) = 7.03, p = .07, it was not significant. There also were no differences in terms of when the different role types first began to imagine tfluynselves as grandparents, x2 (24,N=128) = 26.05, or in their first reaction to the news that they were about to become grandparents, x2(12,N=133) 6.23. Analysis of variance revealed no differences between role types and chronological age F(3,130) = .63, or between role types and the highest grade level completed by the subject, F(3,129) 1.02. In order to determine if role behavior was related to role meaning type a multivariate analysis of variance test (Wilks procedure) was performed using 11 role behaviors and the four role meaning types. No significant differences were found, F(33,337) = 1.22. Given this lack of association between the grandparental role meaning types and various life style variables it was decided that an analysis based on consistent types (prenatal and postnatal consistency) would be performed. Differences, such as those obtained by Robertson, could arise among those subjects who consistently indicated a particular role meaning type. For example, a person would have to be categorized as 130 "Apportioned" both prenatally and postnatally in order to be considered an "Apportioned" role type. To further strengthen the robustness of each type, subjects with mean scores on either the personal or the social dimension, either prenatally or postnatally (means were the same on both occasions) were not included. There was a significant association between prenatal and postnatal role meaning types under these conditions, y2(9,N=41) = 55.9, p < J31. Table 29 shows the pattern of prenatal—postnatal types. As can be seen in Table 29 only 31 of the subjects can actually be classified as consistent role types. Table 30 provides a description of these consistent role types but cell sizes are too small for statistical analysis of life style variables by consistent role types. Table 29: Consistent Role Types Postnatal Role Types App. Ind. Rem. Sym. Prenatal App. n=8 n=1 n:0 n:0 9 Role Ind. n=1 n=5 n=2 n:0 8 Types Rem. n=0 n=1 n=13 n=1 15 Sym. n=2 n=0 n=2 n=5 9 11 7 17 6 41 (App. = Apportioned role type; Ind. = Individualized role type; Rem. = Remote role type; Sym. = Symbolic role type) 131 Table 30: Description of consistent role types Apportioned = 8 :5 men women maternal paternal younger* older** NO‘ ULJUW U'IUO Individualized 3 = 5 men women LION maternal paternal 5.; 2 younger 3 older * 52 years and under ** 53 years and older 132 Symbolic n NW DUN (JUN = 5 men women maternal paternal younger older Remote n 40‘ U103 04 : 13 men women maternal paternal younger older ROLE BEHAVIORS Particular role behaviors anticipated by the grandparents were assessed prenatally and postnatally. Table 31 shows the percentages of grandparents anticipating each behavior. It also shows the actual frequency of the occurrence of each behavior as reported by Robertson (1977). Although a direct comparison cannot be made between the two studies due to the differences in the nature of the samples studied, the differences are provocative. It can be seen that the grandparents in this study anticipate greater involvement with their grandchildren than is reported in the actual behaviors by the subjects :hl Robertson's study. Subjects :hl the current study anticipate taking their grandchildren on vacations and relating family history much more frequently than Robertson's subjects engaged in these behaviors. They are anticipating being advisors to their grandchildren much more frequently than might be predicted given the responses of Robertson's subjects. Overall, when the eXpected role behaviors for subjects in this study are compared to the actual role behaviors as reported by Robertson's grandmothers, the expectant grandparents anticipate greater interaction. 133 Table 31: Role Behavior Pre Post Robertson's Provide gifts 97.7% 98.3% 97.6% Babysit grandchild 86.4% 89.7% 92.0% Engage in home-recreational 95.5% 97.4% 79.2% activities DrOp-in to visit grandchild 53.8% 54.7% 64.8% Relate family history 79.5% 82.1% 47.2% Teach special skills 70.5% 66.7% 42.4%” Help out with family 99.2% 99.1% 39.2% emergencies Take grandchild on vacations 68.9% 72.7% 28.4% Advise grandchild on 43.2% 47.9% 29.6% personal problems Advise grandchild 42.4% 41.0% 24.0% regarding work Advise grandchild 27.2% 31.7% 13.6% regarding religion Frequency of anticipated role behaviors. Information concerning the grandmothers in Robertson's study is adapted from Table 5: Role behaviors with grandchildren (N = 125) from Robertson, 1977. Percentages indicate percentage of subjects anticipating this activity (McGreal) or reporting this activity (Robertson). Prenatal percentages based on Selected Sample (n = 132). Postnatal percentages based on Longitudinal Selected Sample (n = 117). * Robertson's role behavior for this item was labelled "Teach sewing". The wording was changed in the present study to "Teach Special Skills" due to the inclusion of men in the study. 134 SATISFACTION WITH GRANDPARENTHOOD There was considerable variation in what grandparents felt was the most satisfying aspect of grandparenthood; 90.6% of the subjects did specify a particular aspect that they felt was especially satisfying; 3.4% did not know which aspect was most satisfying (the remaining subjects did not list a satisfying aspect of grandparenthood). Many subjects indicated that observing the baby grow and learn was very satisfying and several stressed the enjoyment of this growth process from the standpoint of someone not directly responsible for the child. "Watching his responses to his environment, watching him grow and develOp. Knowing that I am not respnsible for him to the degree I was as a parent". (Grandmother) "Loving without going through the trauma of giving birth. .Appreciating the wonder of a new human being & the changes that take place. Age gives me the distance to see what I didn't necessarily saw (sic) or appreciate at a younger age". (Grandmother) "Being able to enjoy the child and knowing I don't have the responsiblity any longer. I'm tired of the responsibility, esp. when they become teenagers". (Grandfather) "Holding the baby and watching the various stages of development". (Grandfather) 135 Other grandparents emphasized the feelings of family continuity and renewal that emerged with the birth of the first grandchild. "That the next generation is continuing & she [the daughter] values so many ideas & concerns as I did for my children -- she's happy 8: wants more children. one more to love»" (Grandmother) "Holding the baby -- the feeling he's an extension of our daughter (baby of the family)" (Grandmother) "Seeing my daughter extend our values to her daughter. However, not feeling the same obligation as the parent for the grandchildJ' (Grandfather) "The joy of knowing that I brought a life into the world and now that life bore a new life". (Grandfather) Observing the happiness that the baby brought to their children's lives was the most satisfying aspect of grandparent to several subjects. "Loving our granddaughter. Seeing our daughter & son-in-law love her. Seeing our whole family love this little baby". (Grandmother) "Knowing they are happy about having a baby". (Grandmother) "The joy in my daughter and son-in-law" (Grandfather) "My Daughter is Happier Than She's Ever Been". (Grandfather) 136 Finally, just having a grandchild to love was especially satisfying to many grandparents. "Loving our new little personJ' (Grandmother) "JUST FEELING GREAT LOVE FOR MY GRANDSONJ' (Grandmother) "Cuddling, loving my grandchildJ' (Grandfather) "Just loving Him." (Grandfather) When asked which aspects of grandparenthood were least satisfying 53% oftfluasubjects felt that there were none, 17.1% reported that they were not with the baby often enough, 5.1% reported particular infant behaviors that were not satisfying such as the infant's crying, 4.3% reported worry over the baby, and 12.8% reported other aspects (such as "Makes me realize my age" or "My inner annoyance at the other grandmother"). 'L7% of the subjects did not answer this question. 137 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION The Timing of First Grandparenthood The first hypothesis posed the issue cn‘ whether parents who become grandparents "off-time" in terms of their eXpectations for this event have greater difficulty considering themselves as grandparents than do those who become grandparents at the expected time. It is interesting to note the differences in chronological age among those subjects who considered their timing to be "off". Those who felt they were becoming grandparents sooner than expected (aged 42 to 52 years) were in their 40's and early 50's thus providing some support for the hypotheses by Cavan (1962) and Waller and Hill (1951) that many feel the role to be inapprOpriate until after their 40's. Indeed, when asked the best age to become a grandparent the mean was 50 years of age for becoming a grandfather and 49 years of age for becoming a grandmother. But a consideration of age alone would not explain the fact that subjects who were becoming grandparents when expected ranged.in age from 37jyears to 77 years! Among subjects whose chronological age was the same as the age they considered to be the ideal age to 138 become a grandparent, 15.4% were becoming grandparents later than they thought they would, and 7.7% were becoming grandparents sooner than they thought that they would. Thus, factors other than their own age were being considered when a person formed an eXpectation for this event. Since the subjects in this analysis all had married children, the problems involved in being "off-time" due to a pregnancy outside of marriage were not relevant. Therefore, other factors must be considered when eXplaining these findings. Parents of adult children may contemplate their child's maturity, the length of time that the child has been married, whether or not educational plans have been completed, and other factors of this nature when they begin to anticipate a timing for grandparenthood in their own lives. The hypothesis that there would be a relationship between perceiving the assumption of the grandparental role as being off-time and reporting difficulty thinking of oneself as a grandparent was not supported. However, one cannot conclude from this finding that there is no relationship between the two variables. Although many parents in the study became grandparents "off-time" in terms of their own expectations for the event, the ordering of the events was normative relative to society's timetable. As noted above, the subjects' children were married adults at the time of the grandchildren's births. 139 It may be that those who become grandparents at a non- normative time according to society do have difficulty in assuming the grandparental role. Recent research concerning the timing of grandparenthood has emphasized becoming a grandmother too soon according to society's norms. The stresses involved when grandparenthood occurs "off-time" have been eXplored in the mothers of teenage parents (Lee, 1982; Lee and Colletta, 1983; Lee, 1984). A feeling of burden caused by the needs.of the daughter and grandchild was expressed by two-thirds of the subjects. Those who did not feel burden usually had social support systems to aid them, positive self-concepts, and a sense of control over their own lives. Not surprisingly, the younger the mother, the more burden felt by the grandmother. Since difficulty perceiving oneself as ea grandparent was not explained in terms of the timing of the event, other factors need to be considered. Difficulty perceiving oneself as a grandparent also was not due to the fact that the grandchild had not yet been born at the time the subject first participated in the study since 25% of the grandfathers and 15% of the grandmothers had difficulty thinking of themselves in this role even after the grandchild's birth. This result is not due to the fact that many of the grandparents had not yet had the opportunity to see their grandchild; although many lived at considerable distances from the grandchild all but two 140 grandparents had seen their grandchild by the time the postnatal assessment was prove valuable in describing grandparental role meaning types, these classifications should be consistent. However, the inconsistencies may prove interesting in relation to transitions in adult development. It may be that these types reflect issues, such as the transition to middle age, that are undergoing change as an individual becomes a grandparent. Role Behaviors The sixth research question addressed the issue of whether expectant grandparents would anticipate engaging in role behaviors that are performed by experienced grandparents (as reported by Robertson, 1977). The results reveal that when adults are questioned before and after the birth of the first grandchild about the activities that they will perform with grandchildren they anticipate higher levels of interaction than has actually been reported by experienced grandparents. Some differences may be based on realistic assessments of the current intergenerational relationships between parent and adult child. Over time the interactions may be reduced. For example, the fact that over 99% of the subjects in this study reported that they would help out in family emergencies may be influenced by the fact that many helped 159 out at the time of birth. Also, these subjects are substantially younger than the experienced grandmothers studied by Robertson (1977) and therefore may be in a better position to help out both financially and physically. Robertson reports that over 38% of her subjects were over 70 years of age; in the current study 1.8% of the subjects were over 70-years-old (Longitudinal Selected Sample). Another difference between the subjects in the current study and in Robertson's study involves the SES of the two samples. Incomes reported by Robertson were 25% over $9,000, 50% between7$3,000. and $9,000, and 25% under $3,000. [as reported in interviews in 1970]. In contrast, subjects in the current investigation reported high levels of income; over half of the subjects reported incomes over $30,000. [as reported:h11982-1983]. Over 50% of Robertson's subjects had an 8th grade education or less whereas in the current study the average education was beyond high school. When the expectations for role behaviors obtained prenatally and postnatally were compared no differences were found. It should be noted though that as of yet the grandchildren were young and so opportunities for many of the activities had not yet occurred. It would be interesting in further research to follow subjects longitudinally to determine if grandparents actually do engage in these role behaviors as anticipated and the frequency of each behavior. It is likely, however, that 160 expectations for the role may change over time as the grandparents and grandchildren interact and as interactions change with the adult children who have assumed a parental role. Given the discrepancies between the expectations for grandparenthood and the actual behaviors reported by grandparents there is a need to consider the expectations of the child's parents in order to determine if their attitudes are the mediating variables causing the reduction in grandparent involvement. Studies focusing (M1 grandparent-adult child interactions are necessary to gain information on family relationships involving the grandchildren. This is a difficult topic to research due to the need to include members of three-generations in sufficient numbers so that analyses can be performed to determine the effects of sex and lineage. Nevertheless, it .0p 4 appears to be an important issue to investigate based on the findings of this research. Satisfaction with Grandparenthood The final research issue eXplored the aspects of grandparenthood that are considered the most and least satisfying during the grandchild's infancy. More subjects in the current study expressed satisfaction than would be anticipated from the findings of Neugarten and Weinstein (1965). They found that 59% of the grandmothers and 61% of the grandfathers eXpressed comfort and pleasure in the 161 role. Grandparenthood is viewed as a satisfying experience by 90% of the subjects in this study. Over half felt that there were no dissatisfying aspects of the role; the most common source of dissatisfaction among those who specified a least satisfying aspect was not being able to spend enough time with the infant. Grandparents reported that satisfaction with the grandchild was obtained by enjoying the development of the baby, by feelings of family continuity, by observation of the happiness of the new parents, and simply by having another family member to love. Once again, longitudinal investigations would be useful tn) explore how satisfaction derived from grandparenthood changes as the grandchild matures. The above sources of satisfaction are basically uni- directional. It would be interesting to determine whether reciprocity between grandparent and grandchild becomes important in deriving satisfaction from the role once the grandchild reaches a particular point in development. Summary The current investigation examined individuals' expectations for the grandparenting role before and after the birth (H? the first grandchild. Grandparents' perceptions of the timing of this event in terms of their expected time-table for becoming a grandparent also were studied. The results showed considerable variation in the chronological ages of those becoming grandparents at the 162 eXpected time. Plans to engage in various activities with the grandchild varied as a function of perceived timing of the role. Those who were becoming grandparents "off-time", however, were not more likely than "on-time" grandparents to report difficulty thinking of themselves in this role. Contrary to previous findings there were no age differences either prenatally or postnatally on the cluster variables (H' on anticipated role behaviors. Age differences were found with respect to the timing of grandparenthood such that older grandparents reported that the birth of the first grandchild occurred later than expected. Older grandparents also indicated that they perceived themselves as members of an older age-status groups than did younger grandparents. Prenatal and postnatal comparisons showed that, in general, subjects reported an older age status after the birth of the grandchild. The findings reflect a need to consider issues of lineage when investigating the significance of grandparenthood. The prenatal results support the hypothesis that women perceive biological renewal through their daughters whereas men view it as occurring through their sons. However, no relationship between sex and lineage was found when the postnatal data were analyzed for themes of biological renewal. Maternal grandparents were more likely than paternal grandparents to state that they 163 would help the new parents after the birth; paternal grandparents indicated a willingness to help only if needed more often than (H41 maternal grandparents. Lineage differences were evident in early grandparent-grandchild interactions such that maternal grandparents usually saw the infant at a younger age than did the paternal grandparents. However, there were no differences in the frequency of visits after the birth. In general, sex differences were more evident before the birth of the grandchild. Women began to visualize themselves as grandparents at earlier points in their lives than did men. Before the birth men were more likely to perceive themselves as distant from the grandchild whereas women perceived themselves as involved with the grandchild. After the birth there were no sex differences on these variables. Differences remained, however, in regard to helping behaviors such that women were more likely to assist the new parents. Factor analysis replicated the personal and social dimensions of grandparenthood as defined by Robertson (1977). Loadings on the social dimension were inflated by a common error term and therefore the value of dividing subjects on this dimension is questionable. When subjects were assigned to four role-types the types were not related to sex, lineage, age, education or to anticipated role behaviors. 164 The majority of subjects were satisfied with the grandparental role during their grandchild's infancy. Major sources of satisfaction were watching the develOpment of the baby without the responsibility of parenting, feeling a sense of family continuity, observing the happiness of their own children, and having another family member to love. Most grandparents felt that there were no aspects of the role that were dissatisfying. Among those who did express some dissatisfaction the aspects that were not satisfying included not being with the baby often enough, particular infant behaviors, and worry over the baby. The birth of the first grandchild has been shown to be a significant event in the lives of the grandparents. The impact of this event on the adult's life and on the grandchild's develOpment needs to be considered in greater detail. Middle-aged life events, including the transition to grandparenthood, have long been ignored. Longitudinal research of the grandparent-parent-grandchild relationship should add to our understanding of psychosocial develOpment ix: adulthood and intergenerational relationships within the family. The tOpic of grandparenthood promises to be an exciting area of study to those interested in life-span developmental issues. 165 Appendices Appendix A CorreSpondance with Subjects Dear , I would like to invite you to participate in a study of grandparenting attitudes and behaviors. I am studying the reactions to grandparenthood in those who are becoming grandparents for the first time. I am also interested in the attitudes of adults who have married children but as of yet have no grandchildren. Although much is known about the transition to parenthood, very little is known about the transitionto grandparenthood, and what changes, if any, it brings to a person's life. When grandparenthood has been studied most of the participants have been older persons who have been grandparents for many years. This will be the first study, to my knowledge, that investigates the significance of grandparenthood when the first grandchild is born. The study includes: 1. Filling (Mn; a questionnaire about your attitudes toward grandparenting and your expectations for this grandchild before the birth of the child. 2. Filling out a similar questionnaire after the birth of your grandchild. All questionnaires will be mailed to your home and can be completed at your convenience. All data that are collected are strictly confidential and you will remain anonymous. If you are interested in participating in this study please sign the enclosed consent form and fill in the questionnaire. They can be sent to me in the business reply envelOpe. If you would like more information about the study I can be reached at (517) 353-5324 or messages for me can be left at (517) 353-8691. I hope that you will consider participating in this study. Thank you, Cathleen McGreal, M.A. 166 Dear , Thank you for participating in the Grandparenting Project. I am enclosing the second questionnaire that will be used to» compare your attitudes concerning grandparenthood before and after the birth of your grandchild. I thought you might be interested in hearing about some of the other peOple who are in this study. 66 men are participating in the study. Some are as young as 40 but the oldest grandfather is 77. All have just become grandfathers for the first time. 35 of the men have daughters who gave birth to the first grandchild, 29 became grandfathers through their sons. The grandmothers ranged in age from 27 (one of the four step-mothers in the study) to age 71. It is interesting that some of the grandparents in the study are younger than some of the couples becoming parents for the first time! 45 of the women were related to the grandchild through their daughters, 34 through their sons. Many of the people in the study live far away from their grandchildren. Although most of the grandchildren live in Michigan, questionnaires were received from peOple from New York to California, from Florida to Illinois, In fact, 2 of the couples in the study are living as far away as Korea and Mexico! Thank you once again, Cathleen McGreal, M.A. 167 Michigan State University Department of Psychology Grandparenting Project Informed Consent Statement I have freely consented to take part in a study of grandparenting attitudes being conducted by Cathleen McGreal under the supervision of Professor Hiram Fitzgerald both of the Department of Psychology, Michigan State University. The study has been described to me and I understand the explanation that has been given and what any participation will involve. I understand that I EHO free to discontinue participation in the study at any time without penalty. I understand that the results of the study will be treated in strict confidence and that I will remain anonymous. Within these restrictions, results of the study will be made available to me at my request. I understand that my participation in the study does not guarantee any beneficial results to me. I understand that at my request, I can receive additional eXplanation of the study after my participation is completed, but that procedures used to assure confidentiality prevents the release of individual results. Signed: Date: Do you wish to receive summaries of research reports that will be develOped from the results of this study? Yes No 168 Appendix B Sample Questionnaires GRANDPARENTING PROJECT GRANDMOTHER'S PRENATAL QUESTIONNAIRE NOTE: THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS TO BE FILLED OUT BEFORE THE BIRTH OF YOUR GRANDCHILD DIRECTIONS: THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONNAIRE CONCERNS YOUR ATTITUDES TOWARD BECOMING A GRANDMOTHER. PLEASE FILL OUT THE QUESTIONNAIRE INDIVIDUALLY, MEN AND NOMEN OFTEN HAVE DIFFERENT REACTIONS T0 BECOMING GRANDPARENTS! READ EACH ITEM CAREFULLY AND THEN ANSWER. THE FIRST PART OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE CONSISTS OF MULTIPLE-CHOICE ITEMS. PLEASE PLACE AN ”X" IN THE BOX THAT BEST DESCRIBES YOUR FEELINGS. THE SECOND PART OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE CONSISTS OF A GROUP OF QUESTIONS THAT ASK YOU TO WRITE IN YOUR OWN INDIVIDUAL RESPONSE. ALL THE INFORMATION IN THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS CONFIDENTIAL. IT HILL ONLY BE HANDLED BY THE RESEARCH STAFF AND NO NAMES WILL BE ATTACHED. WHEN YOU HAVE COMPLETED THIS QUESTIONNAIRE PLEASE MAIL IT IN THE BUSINESS REPLY ENVELOPE. Subject I.D. (1-3) Card No. 01 Date (4-5) (6-11) Lype 2 (12) ‘169 GRANDPARENTING PROJECT GRANDFATHER'S PRENATAL QUESTIONNAIRE NOTE: THIS QUESTIONIAIRE Is To BE FILLED OUT BEFORE THE BIRTH OF YOUR GRANDCHILD. DIRECTIONS: THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONNAIRE CONCERNS YOUR ATTITUDES TOWARD BECOMING A GRANDFATHER. PLEASE FILL OUT THE QUESTIOINAIRE INDIVIDUALLY, MEN AND WOMEN OFTEN HAVE DIFFERENT REACTIONS TO BECOMING GRANDPARENTS! READ EACH ITEM CAREFULLY AND THEN ANSWER. THE FIRST PART OF THE QUESTIOhNAIRE CONSISTS OF MULTIPLE-CHOICE ITEMS. PLEASE PLACE AN "X" IN THE BOX THAT BEST DESCRIBES YOUR FEELINGS. THE SECOND PART OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE CONSISTS OF A GROUP OF QUESTIONS THAT ASK YOU TO WRITE IN YOUR INDIVIDUAL RESPONSE. ALL THE INFORMATION IN THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS CONFIDENTIAL. IT WILL ONLY BE HANDLED BY THE RESEARCH STAFF ADD ho IMMES WILL BE ATTACHED. WHEN YOU HAVE COMPLETED THIS QUESTIONNAIRE PLEASE MAIL IT IN THE BUSINESS REPLY ENVELOPE. Subject I.D. (1-3) Card No. 01 Date (4-5) 6-11 Type I (12) 170 PAGE 1 SUBJECT # PLEASE INDICATE WHETHER YOU STRONGLY AGREE. AGREE. DISAGREE, 0R STRONGLY DISAGREE HITH EACH OF TIE STATEMENTS BELOH BY PLACING AN "X" IN THE APPROPRIATE BOX. 1. THIS GRANDCHILD IS IMPORTANT TO ME BECAUSE HE OR SHE HILL BE CARRYING ON THE FAMILY LINE. (13) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE GRAPDPARENTS SHOULD MAINTAIN A CONSTANT INTEREST IN THEIR GRAPDOTILDREN. BUT SHOULD ALSO BE CAREFUL TO LEAVE PARENTING STRICTLY TO THE PARENTS. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (14> ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I THINK THAT I HILL FEEL YOUNG AGAIN THROUGH MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (15) ( ) AGREE () DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 171 PAGE 2 SUBJECT # __ 4. THE GRANDPARENT AND GRANDCHILD SHOULD BOTH GET FUN OUT OF THEIR RELATIONSHIP. (16) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I PLAN TO SET MONEY ASIDE ESPECIALLY FOR MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (17) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I FEEL THAT I HILL BE A BETTER GRANDPARENT THAN I HAS A PARENT. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (18) () AGREE () DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE IF THE CHILDREN REQUEST HELP IN CARING FOR THE GRANDOULD ON A REGULAR BASIS IT IS THE GRANDPARENT'S OBLIGATION TO HELP OUT. (19) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 172 PAGE 3 SUBJECT # 8. 10. 11. I D ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE THE GREATEST HAPPINESS Is FOUND IN A FAMILY HHERE ALL MEMBERS WORK TOGETHER AS A GROUP. (37) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE AS THEY GROH OLDER I EXPECT TO ADVISE MY GRANDCHILDREN REGARDING RELIGION. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (38) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE THE MOST IMPORTANT THING ABOUT HAVING GRAIDCHILDREN FOR ME IS THAT THEY HILL BRING A DEEP SENSE OF EMOTIONAL SATISFACTION TO MY LIFE. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (39) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 177 PAGE 8 SUBJECT # 28. 30. 31. OVE OF THE MOST IMPORTANT THINGS THAT I HANT FROM MY GRANDCHILDREN IS FOR THEM TO ”RESPECT THEIR ELDERS". (Ac) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE IT IS IMPORTANT FOR GRANDPARENTS TO LIVE IN A CLOSE GEOGRAPHICAL LOCATION TO THEIR GRANDCHILDREN. (AI) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE GRANDPARENTS SHOULD BE INVOLVED IN THE DAILY LIVES OF THEIR GRANDCHILDREN. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (42) () AGREE () DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE IT IS DIFFICULT T0 THINK OF MYSELF AS A GRANDPARENT. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (43) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 178 PAGE 9 SUBJECT # PLEASE PLACE AN ”X” IN THE BOX THAT YOU FEEL BEST ANSHERS THE QUESTION. 32. WHEN I THINK ABOUT THE AGE THAT I D(PECTED TO BE HHEN I BECAME A GRANDPARENT. I REALIZE THAT I HILL BECOME ONE: (41.) () SOONERTHANIHADEXPECTED ( ) AT ABOUT THE TIME I HAD EXPECTED () IATERTHANIHADEXPECTED 33. I CONSIDER MYSELF TO BE: () YOUNG (45) () MIDDLE-AGED () OLD 34. AS A GRANDPARENT I EXPECT TO PROVIDE GIFTS FOR MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES “6) ()NO i IF YES: A. HOV OFTEN HILL YOU BE PROVIDING GIFTS? B. WHAT KINDS OF GIFTS DO YOU PLAN TO PROVIDE? 179 PAGE 10 SUBJECT # 35. I PLAN TO BABYSIT HITH MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES (47) ()No 9 IF YES: HOH OFTEN DO YOU PLAN TO BABYSIT? 36. I EXPECT TO ENGAGE IN HOME RECRaTIONAL ACTIVITIES (SUCH AS READING STORIES AND PLAYING GAMES) HITH MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES (48) ()NO XI, IF YES: A. How OFTEN HILL YOU BEENGAGED IN THIS TYPE OF ACTIVITY? B. WHAT TYPES OF ACTIVITIES DO YOU PLAN TO DO HITH YOUR GRANDCHILD? 180 PAGE ll SUBJECT # 37. I HILL SPONTANEOUSLY DROP-IN TO VISIT MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES (49) ( ) It) :l’ IF YES: HOH OFTEN DO YOU THINK YOU HILL DROP-IN TO VISIT? 38. ONE OF MY ACTIVITIES WITH MY GRANDCHILD WILL BE TO RELATE FAMILY HISTORY. (50) ( ) YES ( ) 1k) :1’ IF YES: HOH OFTEN HILL YOU TELL YOUR GRANDCHILD ABOUT YOUR FAMILY HISTORY? 39. AS THEY GROH OLDER I PLAN TO TEACH MY GRANDCHILDREN SPECIAL SKILLS (SUCH AS SEHING OR CARPENTRY). (51) ( ) YES ( ) 1k) \lfi IF YES: WHAT SKILLS HILL YOU TEACH YOUR GRANDCHILDREN? 181 PAGE 12 PLEASE HRITE YOUR ANSNER IN TIE BLANK NEXT TO EACH QUESTION. 40. WHAT IS THE BEST AGE FOR A MAN TO BE HIEN HE GETS MARRIED? (52-53) 41. WHAT IS TIE BEST AGE FOR A HOMAN TO BE HEN SIE GETS MARRIED? (SA-55) 12. WHAT Is THE IDEAL AGE FOR A MAN TO BECOME A FATHER? (56-57) 43. WHAT IS THE IDEAL AGE FOR A HOMAN TO BECCME A MOTHER? (58-59) 44. WHAT IS THE IDEAL AGE FOR A MAN TO BECOME A GRANDFATHER? ( 6 ) 60- l 45. WHAT IS THE IDEAL AGE FOR A ROMAN TO BECOME A GRANIMOTHER? (62-63) 46. WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND HHEN YOU THINK OF A 'YOUNG’ MAN? (64-65) 47. WHAT AGE GOES TO YOUR MIND HHEN YOU THINK OF A ’MIDDLE-AGED’ MAN? (66-67) 48. WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND HHEN YOU THINK OF AN ’OLD' MAN? (68-69) 49. WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND HIEN YOU THINK OF A 'YOUNG’ WAN? (70-71) 50. WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND IHEN YOU THINK OF A ’MIDDLE-AGED’ HOMAN? (72-73) 51. WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND THEN YOU THINK OF AN ’OLD’ ROMAN? (74-75) 182 PAGE 13 SUBJECT # THE FOLLOHING QUESTIONS ARE ABOUT YOUR OHN REACTIONS T0 BECOMING A GRAIDPARENT. IFYQJNEEDRDREROO‘ITOANSWERTHBWPLEASEUSEMEXTRAPAGESATTHEENDW THE (IJESTICNNAIRE BOOKLET. 52. HEN DID YOU FIRST BEGIN TO IMAGINE WHAT IT VOULD BE LIKE TO BE A GRANDPARENT? 53. ”HEN DID YOU LEARN THAT YOU WERE ABOUT TO BECCME A GRANDPARENT? POH DID YOU FIND OUT? WHO TOLD YOU? 183 PAGE 14 SUBJECT # 54. IF THIS HAS A PIANVED PREGNANCY, DID YOUR CHILDRBI LET YOU KNOH THAT THEY HERE PLANNING TO HAVE A BABY? HOH DID TIEY LET YOU mm? 55. ARE YOU PLANNING TO HELP OUT IN ANY HAY HHEN THE BABY IS BORN? IF SO. WHAT SPECIFIC THINGS HILL YOU DO TO IELP OUT? 184 PAGE 15 SUBJECT # 56. D0 YOUR PLANS DEPEND ON HHAT YOUR CHILD’S IN-LAHS ARE PLANNING FOR THE BIRTH OF THE BABY? IF SO. HON? 57. HAVE YOU DISCUSSED ANY OF TIE PIANS HITH YOUR CHILD? 58. DO YOU HAVE A PREFERENCE AS TO THE SD< OF THE BABY? IF S0. DO YOU PREFER A BOY OR A GIRL? 185 PAGE 16 SUBJECT # 59. DO YOU KNOH HHAT YOUR CHILDREN ARE PLANNING TO NAME THE BABY? ( ) YES IF IT IS A GIRL: IF IT Is A BOY: ()No 60. HAVE THEY DISCUSSED POSSIBLE NAMES HITH YOU? 61. HAVE YOU SUGGESTED NAMES T0 TIEM? HAVE YOU SUGGESTED THAT TI-E BABY BE NAMED AFTER ANYONE? IF SO. Hm? 62. WHY YOU WERE CI-DOSIMS A NAME FOR YOUR GIN CHILD DID YOUR PARENTS MAKE ANY SUGGESTIOIS? WERE YOU INFLUENCED BY TIEIR SLGGESTIO‘IS IN YOUR CIDICE? 186 PAGE 17 SUBJECT # 63. WHAT HAS YURWHEN YOU FOLNDOUTTHATYOU HERE GOING TO BEAGRANDPARENT’? 04. WHAT HAS YOUR REACTION ONCE THERE HAS TIME FOR THE NEHS TO ”SINK IN"? 65. How HAVE YOUR FRIENDS REACTa) TO TIE NEWS? 187 PAGE 18 SUBJECT # 66. DID YOU IIAVE GOOD RELATIONSHIPS HITH YOLR OHN GRANDPARENTS? IF SO, HHAT SPECIAL THINGS DO You RB‘IEMBER ABOUT YOLR RELATIONSHIPS? (PLEASE INDICATE HHETHER THE MEMORIES ARE OF YOUR MOTIER'S MOTHER. MOTIER’S FATTER. FATHER’S MOTHER OR FATHER’S FATHER.) 188 PAGE 19 SUBJECT # 62. PLEASE ADD ANY ADDITIONAL COMMENTS THAT YOU COULD OFFER CONCERNING YOUR FEELINGS ABOUT GRANDPARENTHOOD. ARE THERE ANY QUESTIONS THAT YOU THINK SHOULD HAVE BEEN ASKED BUT HEREN’T? 189 PAGE 20 SUBJECT # W TIE INFORMATIG‘! REQUESTED (IN THIS PAGE WILL BE USED TO REPORT TI‘E GENERAL CHARACTERISTICS OF TIE AWLTS IN THIS STUDY. ONLY GROUP RESULTS WILL BE PUBLISHED AND THE IDENTITY 0F INDIVIIIJAL SUBJECTS REMAINS AIDNYI’OUS. ALL INFW‘IATIG‘I PROVIDED (N THIS PAGE WILL BE KEPT STRICTLY CCNFIDB‘ITIAL. AGE: Sac OCCUPATION: ETINIC GROUP: WILL THIS GRANDCHILD BE THE CHILD OF YOUR: ( ) BIOLOGICAL CHILD ( ) ADOPTEID CHILD ( ) STEPCHILD WHAT IS YOUR CURRENT MARITAL STATUS: () MARRIED () DIVORCE) () WIDOWED () SINGLE () SEPARATEID PLEASE CIRCLE THE HIGFEST GRADE LEVEL THAT YOU HAVE C(NPLETED: GRADE SCHOOL 5 6 7 8 HIGH SCHOOL 9 10 ll 12 COLLEGE 1.3 14 15 16 DEGREE: GRADUATE WORK 17 18 19 20 DEGREE: 190 PAGE 21 SUBJECT # WHICH OF THE FOLLOWING INCOYLE GROUPS BEST DESCRIBES YOUR FAMILY’S INCOWE? ( ) UNDER $5,(II) ( ) $5,(IIJ - SIOIIII) ( ) $10,(IIJ - $15,011 ( ) $15AIIJ - $20,011 ( ) OVER $ZO,CID AT THE TIME OF BIRTH, HOW FAR WILL YOU LIVE FROI YOLR GRANDCHILD? ( ) IN THE SAME HOUSEHOLD ( ) WITHIN WALKING DISTANCE ( ) WITHIN DAILY DRIVING DISTANCE ( ) OVER 100 MILES AWAY ( ) OVER 101) MILES AWAY PLEASE LIST THE AGE; SEQ AND MARITAL STATUS OF ALL YOUR CHILDREN. AGE SEX MARITALDIADIS HOW OLD WERE YOU WHEN YOU FIRST GOT MARRIED? ’ HOW OLD WERE YOU WHEN YOU HAD YOUR FIRST CHILD? 191 EARRA PAGES FOR LONG ANSWERS SUBJECT # (PAGE 22) ADDITION To QUESTION # 192 EXTRA PAGES FOR LONG ANSWERS SUBJECT # (PAGE 23) ADDITION TO QUESTION # 1173 ”no I l C GRANDMOTHER'S POSTNATAL QUESTIONNAIRE NOTE: THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS TO BE FILLED OUT AFTER THE BIRTH OF YOUR GRANDCHILD DIRECTIO‘IS: THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONNAIRE Is ABOUT YOUR ATTITUDES TOWARD GRANDPARENan NOW THAT YOU HAVE BECOME A GRANmOTHER. PLEASE FILL OUT THE QLESTIONNAIRE INDIVIDUALLY, GRANDMOTHERS AND GRANDFATHERS OFTEN HAVE DIFFERENT REACTIONS TO BECO‘IING GRANDPARENTS! READ EACH ITBM CAREFULLY AND TEEN ANSWER. THE FIRST PART OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE CONSISTS OF MULTIPLE- CHOICE ITEMS. PLEASE PLACE AN "X” IN THE Box THAT BEST DESCRIBES YQLR FEELINGS. THE SECOND PART OF THE QUESTIONNAIRE OONSISTS OF A GROUP OF QUESTIONS THAT ASK YOU To WRITE IN YOIR INDIVIDUAL RESPONSE. ALL THE INFORMATION IN THE QUESTIONNAIRE IS CONFIDENTIAL. IT WILL ONLY BE HANDLED BY THE RESEARCH STAFF AND NO NAMES WILL BE ATTACHED. WHEN YOU HAVE COMPLETED THIS QUESTIONNAIRE PLEASE MAIL IT IN THE BUSINESS REPLY ENVELOPE. ubject I.D. (1-3) Card No. 01 Date _— (4-5) (6-11) Type 4 (12) 19h DRAW GRANDFATHER'S POSTNATAL QUESTIONNAIRE NOTE: THIS QUESTIONNAIRE IS TO BE FILLED OUT AFTER THE BIRTH OF YOUR GRANDCHILD DIRECTIONS: THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONNAIRE IS ABOUT YOUR ATTITUDES TOWARD GRANDPARENTHOOD NOW THAT YOU HAVE BECOME A GRANDFATHER. PLEASE FILL OUT THE QUESTIONNAIRE INDIVIDUALLY, GRANDFATHERS AND GRANDMOTTERS OFTEN HAVE DIFFERENT REACTIONS TO BECOMING GRANDPARENTS! READ EACH ITEM CAREFULLY AND THEN ANSWER. THE FIRST PART OF THE QUESTIONIAIRE CONSISTS OF MULTIPLE-CHOICE ITEMS. PLEASE PLACE AN "X" IN THE Box THAT BEST DESCRIBES YOUR FEELINGS. THE SECOND PART OF THE QUESTIONIAIRE CONSISTS OF A GROIP OF QUESTIONS THAT ASK YOU TO WRITE IN YOUR INDIVIDUAL RESPONSE. ALL THE INFORMATION IN THE QUESTIONNAIRE IS CONFIDENTIAL. IT WILL ONLY BE HANDLED BY THE RESEARCH STAFF AND NO NAMES WILL BE ATTACHED. WHEN YOU HAVE CCFPLETED THIS QUESTIONNAIRE PLEASE MAIL IT IN THE BUSINESS REPLY ENVELOPE . Subject I.D. (1-3) Card No. 01 Date (4—5) (6-11) Type 3 (12) 195 PAGE 1 SUBJECT # PLEASE INDICATE WHETHER YGJ STRG‘GLY AGREE; AGREE; DISAGREE; (I? STROMSLY DISAGREE WITH EACH OF TI—E STATEMENTS BELOW BY PLACING AN "X” IN THE APPROPRIATE BOX . 1. THIS GRANDCHILD IS IMPORTANT TO ME BECAUSE HE OR SHE WILL BE CARRYING ON THE FAMILY LINE. (13) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE GRANDPARENTS SHOULD MAINTAIN A CONSTANT INTEREST IN THEIR GRANDCHILDREN. BUT SHOULD ALSO BE CAREFUL TO LEAVE PARENTING STRICTLY TO THE PARENTS. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (14) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I THINK THAT I WILL FEEL YOUNG AGAIN TTROUGH MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (15> () AGREE () DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 196 PAGE 2 SUBJECT # __ A. TEE GRANDPARENT AND GRANDCHILD SHOULD BOTH GET FUN OUT OF THEIR RELATIONSHIP. (16) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I PLAN TO SET MONEY ASIDE ESPECIALLY FOR MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (17) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I FEEL THAT I WILL BE A BETTER GRANDPARENT THAN I WAS A PARENT. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (18) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE IF THE CHILDREN REQUEST HELP IN CARING FOR THE GRANDCHILD ON A REGULAR BASIS IT IS THE GRANDPARENT’S OBLIGATION TO HELP OUT. (I9) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 197 PAGE 3 SUBJECT # 8. 10. l EXPECT THAT BECCNIMS A GRANDPARENT WILL HAVE LITTLE EFFECT ON MY LIFE. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (20) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE GRANDPARBTTS SHOULD VISIT THEIR GRANDCHILD FREQUENTLY BUT SHOULD NOT OFFER ADVICE ON CHILDREARING. (21) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE GRANDPARENTHOOD WILL BE A SOURCE OF SATISFACTION SINCE MY GRANDCHILD WILL BE ABLE TO ACCOMPLISH THAT WHICH MY CHILDREN AND I HAVE NOT. (22) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE A GRANDPARENT USUALLY HAS LITTLE CONTACT WITH THE GRANDCHILD EXCEPT ON SPECIAL HOLIDAYS OR BIRTHDAYS. <23) ()hmwmg ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 198 PAGE A SUBJECT # 12. I REALLY DON’T HAVE MJCH TIME TO DEVOTE TO BEING A GRANDPARENT. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (24) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 13. IF BOTH PARaTTS WORK THEN ONE OF TIE GRANDPARENTS SHOULD ASSLME THE CARETAKING OF TIE CHILD. (25) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 114. THE GRAhDPAREJWT-GRANDCHILD RELATIONSHIP SHOULD BE CHARACTERIZED BY INFORMALITY AND PLAYFULNESS. (26) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 15. AS A GRANDPARENT I WILL HELP OUT IN FAMILY EMERGENCIES. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (27) () AGREE () DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 199 PAGE 5 SUBJECT # 16. 18. 19. ONE OF TIE MOST IMPORTANT THINGS ABOUT HAVING GRANDCHILDREN Is THAT THEY PROVIDE ME WITH A WAY TO SEE MY BLOOD LINE CARRIED ON FOR ANOTHER GENERATION. (23) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I PLAN TO TAKE MY GRANDCHILDREN ON VACATIONS WHEN THEY ARE OLDER. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (29) ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE LIFE WOULD BE VERY LONELY FOR ME IF I NEVER HAD GRANDCHILDREN. ( ) STRONGLY AGREE (30) () AGREE () DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE AS TIEY GROW OLDER I EXPECT TO ADVISE MY GRANDCHILDREN ON THEIR PERSONAL PROBLB48. (31) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 200 PAGE 6 SUBJECT # 20. 22. GOING TO VISIT A FRIEND FOR A HOLIDAY IS MORE ENJOYABLE THAN SPENDING HOLIDAYS WITH ONE's FAMILY. (32) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE I THINK THAT I SHOULD BE ABLE TO GIVE MY GRANDCHILD WHATEVER I CAN AND NOT BE WORRIE) ABOUT SPOILING HIM OR HER. <33) ( ) STRONGLY AGREE ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE As THEY GROW OLDER I E ( ) AGREE ( ) DISAGREE ( ) STRONGLY DISAGREE 203 PAGE 9 SUBJECT # ______ PLEASE PLACE AN "X" IN THE Box THAT YOU FEELS BEST ANSWERS THE QUESTION. 32. WHEN I THINK ABOUT THE AGE THAT I EXPECTED TO BE WHEN I BECAME A GRANDPARENT I REALIZE THAT I BECAME ONE: (44) ( ) SOONER THAN I HAD EXPECTED ( ) AT ABOUT THE TIME I HAD EXPECTED ( ) LATER THAN I HAD EXPECTED 33. 1 CONSIDER MYSELF TO BE: ( ) YOUNG (45) ( ) MIDDLE-AGED ( ) OLD 3A. AS A GRANDPARENT I EXPECT TO PROVIDE GIFTS FOR MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES ___ - <46) ( ) NO IF YES: A. HOW OFTEN WILL YOU BE PROVIDING GIFTS? 8. WHAT KINDS OF GIFTS DO YOU PLAN TO PROVIDE? 20h PAGE 10 SUBJECT # 35. I PLAN TO BABYSIT WITH MY GRANDCHILD. () YES (47) () NO {V IF YES: HOW OFTEN DO YOU PLAN TO BABYSIT? 36. I EXPECT TO ENGAGE IN PINE RECREATIONAL ACTIVITIES (SUCH AS READING STORIES AND PLAYING GAMES) WITH MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES (48) ()NO 9 IF YES: A. HOW OFTEN WILL YOU BE‘ENGAGED IN THIS TYPE OF ACTIVITY? B. WHAT TYPES OF ACTIVITIES DO YOU PLAN TO DO WITH YOUR GRANDCHILD? 205 PAGE 11 SUBJECT # 37. I WILL SPONTANEOUSLY DROP-IN TO VISIT MY GRANDCHILD. ( ) YES (49) ( ) IL) ;I‘ IF YES: HOW OFTEN Do YOU THINK YOU WILL DROP-IN TO VISIT? 38. ONE OF MY ACTIVITIES WITH MY GRANDCHILD WILL BE TO RELATE FAMILY HISTORY. (50) ( ) YES ( ) II) :;1’ IF YES: HOW OFTEN WILL YOU TELL YOUR GRANDCHILD ABOUT YOUR FAMILY HISTORY? 39. AS THEY GROW OLDER I PLAN TO TEACH MY GRANDCHILDREN SPECIAL SKILLS (SUCH As SEWING OR CARPENTRY). (51) ( ) YES ( ) It) Slfi IF YES: WHAT SKILLS WILL YOU TEACH YOUR GRANDCHILDREN? 206 PAGE 12 PLEASE IRITE YOUR ANSNER IN TIE BLANK NEXT TO EACH QJESTIGW. ‘40. THAT IS THE BEST AGE FOR A MAN TO BE WEN HE GETS MARRIED? (52-53) WHAT IS TIE BEST AGE FOR A WOMAN To BE WEN STE GETS MARRIED? (SA-55) WHAT Is TIE IDEAL AGE FOR A MAN TO BECOME A FATHER? (56-57) WHAT IS TIE IDEAL AGE FOR A KHAN TO BECOME A MOTHER? (58-59) WHAT IS THE IDEAL AGE FOR A MAN TO BECOME A GRANDFATHER? (6 ) 0-61 WHAT IS THE IDEAL AGE FOR A W TO BECCNE A GRANIMOTHER? (62-63) WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND WHEN YOU THINK OF A ’YOUNG’ MAN? (64-65) WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND WTEN YOU THINK OF A ’MIDDLE-AGED’ MAN? (66—67) WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND WrEN YOU THINK OF AN ’OLD’ MAN? (68-69) WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND WIEN YOU THINK OF A 'YOUNG’ WOMAN? (70-71) WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND WHEN YOU THINK OF A 'MIDDLE-AGED’ WOMAN? (72-73) WHAT AGE COMES TO YOUR MIND WHEN YOU THINK OF AN ’OLD’ WOMAN? (74-75) 207 PAGE 13 THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS ARE ABOUT YOUR OWN REACTIONS TO BECCNING A GRANDPARENT. 52. HOW DID YOU LEARN THAT YOUR GRANDCHILD HAD BEEN BORN? WHO TOLD YOU? 3. WERE YOU AT THE HOSPITAL DURING THE BIRTH OF YOUR GRANDCHILD? IF SO, COULD YOU DESCRIBE YOUR EXPERIENCES THERE? 5A. HAVE YOU SEEN YOUR GRANDCHILD YET? IF YOU HAVE, HOW OLD WAS YOUR GRANDCHILD WHEN YOU FIRST SAW HIM OR HER? 55. DID YOU STAY WITH THE PARENTS AND BABY AFTER THE BIRTH? IF SO,INNILONG DID YOU STAY? 56. DID YOU HELP OUT IN ANY WAY AFTER THE BABY WAS BORN? IF SO, WHAT DID YOU DO TO HELP OUT? 2CW3 PAGE 1A 57. WHAT WAS YOUR FIRST REACTION WHEN YOU FOUID OUT YOU WERE A GRANDPARENT? 58. HOW DO YOU FEEL ABOUT THE NAME THAT WAS GIVEN TO YOUR GRANDCHILD? 59. IS THE BABY THE SEX YOU HAD MOPED IT WOULD BE? 60. HOW OFTEN DO YOU SEE YOUR GRANDCHILD? HOW OFTEN DO YOU EXPECT TO SEE HIM OR HER DURING THE FIRST YEAR OF LIFE? 61. ARE YOU AS INVOLVED WITH THIS GRANDCHILD AS YOU HAD EXPECTED TO BE? IF NOT, WHY DO YOU THINK YOU AREN'T AS INVOLVED AS YOU THOUGHT YOU WOULD BE? 62. HAS THIS EXPERIENCE CHANGED YOUR RELATNIUNNP WITH YOUR CHILDREN? IF SO, WHAT ARE THE CHANGES? ARE THEY POSITIVE OR NEGATIVE CHANGES? 209 PAGE 15 63. HAS THIS EXPERIENCE CHANGED YOUR RELATIONSHIP WITH YOUR SPOUSE? IF SO, WHAT CHANGES HAVE OCCURRED? ON. HOW HAVE YOLR FRIENDS REACTED TO TIE NEWS THAT YOU ARE NOW A GRANDPARENT? 65. DO YOU HAVE A GOOD RELATIONSHIP WITH THE OTHER SET OF GRANDPARENTS? HAVE ANY CONFLICTS OR JEALOUSIES ARISEN OVER YOUR GRANDCHILD? ER . DID YOUR PARENTSI OR IN-LAWS' BEI'AVIORS AS GRANDPARENTS INFLUENCE HOW YOU FEEL GRANDPARENTS SHOULD ACT? IF SO, HOW? 67. WHAT ASPECTS OF GRANDPARENTHOCD DO YOU FIND THE POST SATISFYING? 210 PAGE 16 68. WHAT ASPECTS OF GRANDPARENTHOOD DO YOU FIND THE LEAST SATISFYING? 69. IS BEING A GRANDPARENT DIFFERENT IN ANY WAY FRON WHAT YOU HAD EXPECTED? 70. RECENTLY THERE HAS BEEN PUBLICITY ABOUT ”GRANDPARENTS’ RIGHTS”, IN PARTICULAR, THE RIGHT OF THE GRANDPARENTS TO VISIT AND CORRESPOND WITH GRANDCHILDREN AFTER THE PARENTS ARE DIVORCED OR AFTER A PARENT DIES AND THE SPOUSE REMARRIES. WHAT RIGHTS DO YOU FEEL YOU ARE ENTITLED TO AS A GRANDPARENT? WHAT WOULD BE THE LEAST AMOUNT OF CONTACT WITH A GRANDCHILD THAT YOU FEEL A GRANDPARENT IS ENTITLED TO? 71. HHAT ADVICE WOULD YOU GIVE TO THOSE WHO ARE ABOUT TO BECO‘IE GRANU‘ARENTS FOR THE FIRST TIME? 211 PAGE 17 72. WHAT SPECIAL MEMORIES OF YOUR OWN GRANDPARENTS HAVE COME TO MIND NOW THAT YOU ARE A GRANDPARENT? 73. IF YOU LIVE FAR AWAY FROM YOUR GRANDCHILD: HOW HAS THE DISTANCE AFFECTED YOUR PERCEPTION 0F GRANDPARENTHOOD? HHAT CHANGES WOULD YOU EXPECT IF YOUR GRANDCHILD LIVED CLOSER? HOW DO YOU MAINTAIN CONTACT WITH YOU? GRANDCHILD? HOW DO YOU FEEL ABOUT BEING SEPARATED FROM YOUR GRANDCHILD? DO YOU HAVE ANYTHING ELSE TO ADD ABOUT THE EFFECTS OF LIVING FAR AWAY FROM YOUR GRANDCH I LD? 212 PAGE 18 WHAT IS YOUR GRANDCHILD’S BIRTH DATE: WHAT IS YOUR GRANDORILD’S SEX? ( ) 1. MALE ( ) 2. FEMALE 90 YOU KNOW YOUR GRANDCHILD’S WEIGHT AT BIRTH? EOLIIDS QINCES DO YOU KNOW YOUR GRANDCHILD’S LENGTH AT BIRTH? INCHES HAS YOUR GRANDCHILD HAD ANY PROLONGED OR GENERAL ILLNESS SINCE BIRTH? ( ) 1. YES ( ) 2. HO IF YES, PLEASE DESCRIBE THE PROBLEM: HOW FAR DO YOU LIVE FROM YOUR GRANDCHILD? IN THE SAME HOUSEHOLD WITHIN WALKING DISTANCE ) ) ) WITHIN DAILY DRIVING DISTANCE ) OVER 100 MILES AWAY ) OVER 1000 MILES AWAY WHAT ARE THE AGES CF YOUR GRANDCHILD'S PARENTS? MOTHER: FATHER: WHAT IS THEIR CLRRENT MARITAL STATUS? ( ) 1. MARRIED ( ) 2. DIVORCED ( ) 3. WIDOWED ( ) A. SINGLE ( ) 5. SEPARATED I \ f‘ I .. .-.-\ 2JJ3 PAGE 19 SUBJECT # IAOIRNID HEORNTKN ALL INFORIATHJICWITHIS PAGE WILL BE KEPT STRICTLY CONFIDENTIAL. WHAT IS YOUR CURRENT MARITAL STATUS:> ( l. MARRIED ( ) 2. DIVORCED ( ) 3. WIDOWED ( ) A. SINGLE ( ) 5. SEPARATED WHAT IS YOUR AGE? WHAT IS YOUR RACIAL OR ETHNIC BACKGROUND? ) 1. WHITE ( ) 2. BLACK ( ) 3. HISPANIC ( ) A. AMERICAN INDIAN ( ) 5. ORIENTAL ( ) 6. OTHER (PLEASE SPECIFY) WHICH OF THE FOLLOWING INCOME GROUPS BEST DESCRIBES YOUR FAMILY'S CURRENT INCOME? ) l. UNDER $5,000 ( ) 2. $5,000 — $10,000 ( ) 3. $10,000 - $15,000 ( ) A. $15,000 - $20,000 ( ) 5. $20,000 - $25,000 ( ) 6. $25,000 - $30,000 ( ) 7. $30,000 - $35,000 ( ) 8. $35,000 - $A0,000 ( ) D. (AER NALOOO WHAT IS YOUR OCCUPATION? (IF YOU ARE RETIRED, WHAT WAS YOUR OCCUPATION BEFORE RETIREMENT?) 21}; Appendix C Questionnaire Keys Questionnaire Key: Question Number 1 1O 11 12 13 1A 15 Source Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Robertson and and and and and and and and and and and and and and Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein 215 Prenatal Questionnaires Purpose Significance of Role: Biological Renewal and Continuity Style of grandparent: Formal Significance of Role: Biological Renewal and Continuity Style of grandparent: Funseeker Significance of Role: Resource Person Significance of Role: Emotional Self Fulfillment Style of grandparent: Surrogate Parent Significance of Role: Remote Style of grandparent: Formal Significance of Role: Vicarious achievement through grandchild Style of grandparent: Distant Figure Significance of Role: Remote Style of grandparent: Surrogate Parent Style of grandparent: Funseeker Role Behavior Question Number 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 2A 25 26 27 28 29 3O 31 Source Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Kornhaber Kornhaber Original 32 Original 33 3A 35 36 37 38 39 Original Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson and Woodward and Woodward 216 Purpose Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Meaning Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Meaning Significance of Role Significance of Role Degree of comfort in role Timing of grandparenthood: ExDected life history Perceived age status Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Question Number '40 A1 A2 u3 AA us us A7 A8 A9 SO 51 52 53 5A 55 Source Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Original Original Original Original Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore 217 Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Purpose Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms To determine when a person begins to include this event in their expected life history. To determine when expectant parents inform their own parents of the impending birth and to determine how this information is usually conveyed. To determine iJ‘ an "Intent to Conceive" Announcement is made prior to the actual occurrence of pregnancy. To determine whether the expectant grandparent plans to assume a helping role in the family at the time of birth. Question Number 56 57 58 59 6O 61 62 63 Source Original Original Original Original Original Original Original Original 218 Purpose To determine whether plans are made that take into consideration the <>tIIeT‘ seat <>f grandparents. To determine whether plans are made that take into account the needs and desires of the married child. To determine whether there is a preference for a grandchild of a particular sex(relates to <1ue:3ti<>ns of biological renewal as a significant meaning to grandparenthood). To determine whether there is intergener- ational discussion of names. To determine whether there is intergener- ational discussion of names. To determine whether there is intergener- ational discussion of names. To determine whether there is intergener- ational discussion of names. To determine the initial reeac:ti.orI tLO tIIe realization that the grandparental role will soon be assumed. Question Number 6A 65 66 67 Source Original Original Original Original 219 Purpose To determine later reactions to the realization that the grandparental role will soon be assumed. To assess peer reaction to the news To determine whether expectations for the grandparental role are based CHI experiences wi.tII (Inee's oven grandparents. To elicit information on other aspects of grandparenting that were not directly addressed in the questionnaire. Question Number 1 10 11 12 13 1A 15 Source Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Neugarten Robertson Questionnaire Key: and and and and and and and and and and and and and and Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein Weinstein 220 Postnatal Questionnaires Purpose Significance of Role: Biological Renewal and Continuity Style of grandparent: Formal Significance of Role: Biological Renewal and Continuity Style of grandparent: Funseeker Significance of Role: Resource Person Significance of Role: Emotional Self Fulfillment Style of grandparent: Surrogate Parent Significance of Role: Remote Style of grandparent: Formal Significance of Role: Vicarious achievement through grandchild Style of grandparent: Distant Figure Significance of Role: Remote Style of grandparent: Surrogate Parent Style of grandparent: Funseeker Role Behavior Question Number 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 2A 25 26 27 28 29 3O 31 32 33 3A 35 36 37 38 39 Source Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Kornhaber Kornhaber Original Original Original Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson Robertson and Woodward and Woodward 221 Purpose Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Meaning Role Meaning Role Behavior Role Meaning Role Meaning Significance of Role Significance of Role Degree of comfort in role Timing of grandparenthood: Expected life history Perceived age status Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Role behavior, examples Question Number no A1 A2 A3 AA us A6 A7 A8 A9 so 51 52 53 5A 55 Source Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Neugarten, Original Original Original Original Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore Moore 222 Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Lowe Purpose Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms Assessment of age norms To determine how the grandparent had learned of the birth of the grandchild. To determine if the grandparent was at the hospital at the time of the birth. To determine whether the grandparent has seen the grandchild yet, and if so, the age of the grandchild when first seen. To determine whether the grandparent stayed with the new parents after the birth. Question Number 56 57 58 59 6O 61 62 63 6A Source Original Original Original Original Original Original Original Original Original 223 Purpose To determine if the grandparent helped out after the birth and if so what the grandparent did to help. To determine the grandparent's first reaction to the news that she/he had become a grandparent. To determine the grandparent's reaction to the name that was given to the grandchild. To determine if the baby was the sex that the gr~ar1dx>ar~er1t head expressed a preference for. To assess frequency of grandparent—grandchild visits during the first year. To determine whether the person's expectations for involvement with the grandchild had been met so far. To determine whether any changes had occurred in the parent-adult child relationship due to this experience. To determine whether any changes had occurred in relationships with one's spouse due ‘to this experience. To assess peer reaction to the role. Question Number 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 Source Original Original Original Original Original Original Original Original Original 224 Purpose To assess the nature of the *relationship with the other set of grandparents. To determine whether the subject's parents' or in-laws' behaviors as grandparents influenced the subject's behavior. To assess what aspects of grandparenthood are considered to be the most satisfying to the new grandparents. To assess what aspects of grandparenthood are considered to be the least satisfying to the new grandparents. To assess whether grandparenthood differs from the person's expectations for the role. To determine if the subject feels entitled to certain rights as a grandparent and if so what rights these are. To elicit advice from the subjects to be offered to others becoming grandparents for the first time. To assess memories of one's own grand- parents. To determine the sub- jects perceptions of how distance affects the grandparent- grandchild relationship. Appendix D Computer Codebook Variable Subject ID Card A Date Type Computer Codebook: Column Card Number of Columns 1-3 u-s 6-11 12 13 1A 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 2A 25 26 27 28 29 3 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 225 Prenatal Questions Missing Key Values Odd = Male Even = Female Date question- naire returned 1 = prenatal grandfather 2 = prenatal grandmother 3 = postnatal grandfather A = postnatal grandmother Key for questions 1 to 31: 1 =strong1y agree agree disagree strongly disagree When more than one response was checked the response was determined by random assignment Missing values: 8 = wrote in an answer 9 = left blank 2 3 u Variable 018 Q19 Q20 Q21 022 023 QZM Q25 Q26 Q27 028 Q30 Q31 Q32 Q33 Column Card Number of 30 31 32 33 3a 35 36 37 38 39 no in 142 u3 “5 Columns 1 1 226 Missing Key Values 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 8,9 1 : Sooner than expected 2 = At about the expected time 3 = Later than expected Missing Values: 8 = Wrote in that they never thought about it 9 = Left blank 1 = Young 2 = Between Young and Middle Aged 3 = Middle Aged u = Between Old and Middle Aged S = Old Variable Q33 (cont) Q3u Q35 Q36 Q37 Q38 Q39 ONO Qu1 ou2 093 OHM 095 096 097 098 099 050 051 Age Sex Occupation Column Card Number of H6 97 :18 “9 50 51 52-55 56-59 60-63 6u—67 68-71 72-75 76-79 13-16 17-20 21-2u 25-28 29—32 33-3u 35 _36-37 Columns 1 8,9 1 8,9 1 8,9 1 8,9 1 8,9 1 8,9 u u u u u s u a u I u u u a u u u n u i u u u a 2 99 1 2 99 227 Missing Key Values Missing Values: 8 : Wrote in that they never thought about it 9 = Left blank Questions 3“ to 39: 1 s 2 ye no Missing Values: 8 = Wrote in answer 9 = Left blank Age ranges appear as written on questionnaire. 'Missing values: 0000 = Left blank 0101 : Wrote in that s/he was not certain 0202 : Wrote in that it depended on the person's maturity 99 = Left blank male 1 2 female 99 = Left blank Variable Ethnic Group Relation 38 39 to Expectant Parent Marital Status Grade Level Income Distance from grandchild Number of Children Age of Youngest Child U0 U1-U2 113 UN “5 u8-u7 Column Card Number of Columns 1 9 99 99 228 Missing Values Key \OCDO‘U‘ 1:de —‘ N \OUU \OWEUUMJ White Black Hispanic American Indian Oriental Other American Left blank biological child adopted child step-child Left blank married divorced widowed single separated Left blank Coded highest Level completed 99 U‘twmfl Nd Left blank under 5,000 5,000—10,000 10,000—1S,OOO 1S,OOO-20,000 over 20,000 Same house Walking distance Daily driving distance Over 100 miles Over 1000 miles Left blank Variable Age of Oldest Child Age of Expectant Parent Sex of Expectant Parent Marital Status of Expectant Parent Subject's Age at Marriage Subject's Age at Birth of First Child Lineage 052 u8—u9 50-51 52 53 59-55 56-57 58 13-111 2 Column Card Number of Columns 2 Missing Key Values 99 99 99 88,99 99 Male Female N u u Married Divorced Widowed Single Separated Left blank \OUlszd 99 = Left blank Missing Values: 88 = No children step-parent 99 = Left blank maternal paternal both (2 children expecting) tum—8 nun 01 = When I was a child 02 = When I was raising my own child- ren When my children began to get married 0“ = During the pregnancy When peers became grandparents 03 O5 Variable 053A 0538 053C QSNA Column Card Number of Missing Columns Values 06 O7 08 09 99 15 3 1 9 1 z: \0U'1 16 3 1 9 WNJ \OUWJ‘: 17 3 1 9 \OU11: WN‘ ELAN—I 230 Key When I reached a particular age When I began filling out questionnaire Have not yet begun to imagine it Other response Left blank When parents- to be found out Within 1st trimester On a particular occasion or date At a particu- lar age Other response Left blank Phone call Written message Given a symbolic gift Told in person Other response Left blank Father-to-be Mother-to-be Both expectant parents Spouse Other person Left blank Yes No Don't know Pregnancy was not planned Left blank Variable Column Card Number of Missing Key Columns Values 0598 19 3 1 9 1 = Directly told of plans 2 = Stated that birth control not being used 3 = Stated that they were having fertility problems 9 = Implied an interest in babies Other response Not applicable Left blank \DO‘U'1 II II II 055 20 3 1 9 Yes No If needed or emergency Don‘t know Left blank WNH II II II \01': II II 056 21 3 1 9 Yes No Perhaps Other response Left blank \1:) .l=? \.A) r\) -‘ II II II II II 057 22 3 1 9 Yes No Spouse has Not in terms of specifics Other response Left blank snow—9 II M II n \OU'I II II 058A 23 3 1 9 Yes, for a girl Yes, for a boy No preference Yes, for a boy and a girl Left blank KW“); II II II II \0 II 0588 2A 3 1 9 Yes, for a girl Yes, for a boy No preference Yes, for a boy and a girl - Left blank .82: kth f‘\) -‘ \l) I 231 Variable 060 061A 0618 062A 062A 063 06k 065 Column 25 26 27 28 29 3O 31 32 Card Number of Missing Columns 1 232 Values 9 01': N—0 \OWNJ \OUUN‘ \OC DUN-i \OUJN-J \OkfitWN—O \OU‘BUJNJ xowzuum-a Key Yes No Other response Left blank Yes No General sugg- stions but no specifics Other response Left blank Yes No Other response Left blank Yes No Other response Left blank Yes No For some children but not for all Other response Left blank Positive Neutral Negative Mixed Other reaction Left blank Positive Neutral Negative Mixed Other reaction Left blank Positive Neutral Negative Mixed Other reaction Left blank Computer Codebook: Postnatal Questions Variable Column Card Number of Missing Key Columns Values Subject ID 1-3 u 3 Odd = Male Even = Female Card # fl-S u 2 Date 6-11 u 6 Date question- nire returned Type 12 u 1 1 = prenatal grandfather 2 = prenatal grandmother 3 = postnatal grandfather u = postnatal grandmother Q1 13 u 1 8,9 Key for questions 1 to 31: 02 1“ u 1 8,9 1 =strongly agree 03 15 u 1 8,9 2 : agree 3 = disagree 09 16 u 1 8,9 N = strongly disagree 05 17 u 1 8,9 When more than one response was 06 18 U 1 8,9 checked the response was 07 19 u 1 8,9 determined by random assignment 08 20 u 1 8,9 Missing values: 8 = wrote in an 09 21 u 1 8,9 answer 9 = left blank Q10 22 A 1 8,9 011 23 u 1 8,9 012 211 u 1 8,9 Q13 25 u 1 8,9 019 26 u 1 8,9 015 27 N 1 8,9 016 28 u 1 8,9 017 29 u 1 8,9 233 Variable Column Card Number of Missing Key Columns Values Q18 30 u 1 8,9 019 31 u 1 8,9 020 32 A 1 8,9 021 33 u 1 8.9 022 314 u 1 8,9 023 35 u 1 8.9 02u 36 u 1 8,9 025 37 u 1 8,9 026 38 u 1 8.9 027 39 u 1 3.9 028 no u 1 8,9 030 H1 u 1 8,9 Q31 142 u 1 8,9 032 “3 u 1 8,9 1 = Sooner than expected 2 = At about the expected time 3 = Later than expected Missing Values: 8 = Wrote in that they never thought about it 9 = Left blank Q33 “5 U 1 8,9 1 : Young 2 : Between Young and Middle Aged 3 = Middle Aged U = Between Old and Middle Aged 5 = Old 234 Variable Column Card Number of Columns 033 (cont) Q3“ 116 11 1 035 “7 u 1 036 “8 U 1 037 “9 u 1 038 50 u 1 039 51 A 1 090 52-55 9 n 081 56-59 a u Qu2 60-63 u u 093 69-67 N u 0““ 68-71 A u 095 72-75 9 u 096 76-79 9 u 097 13-16 5 H 098 17-20 5 u 099 21-2fl 5 u 050 25-28 5 u 051 29-32 5 u Grandchild 33-38 5 6 Birthdate Grandchild 39 5 1 Sex Birthweight NO-U3 5 u 235 Missing Values 999999 9999 Key Missing Values: 8 = Wrote in that they never thought about it 9 = Left blank Questions 3“ to 39: 1 s 2 ye no Missing Values: 8 = Wrote in answer 9 = Left blank Age ranges appear as written on questionnaire. 'Missing values: 0000 = Left blank 0101 = Wrote in that s/he was not certain 0202 = Wrote in that it depended on the person's maturity 999999 = Left blank 1 = Male 2 = Female 9 = Left blank 9999 = Left blank Variable Column Card Number of Length at “u-“7 Birth Prolonged 98 Illness Distance “9 from grandchild Age of 50-51 grandchild's mother Age of 52-53 grandchild's father Marital 5“ status of grandchild's parents Marital 55 status of grandparent Grandparent 56-57 Age Race/Ethnic 58 Group 5 Columns Values N 9999 1 9 1 9 2 99 2 99 1 9 1 9 2 99 1 9 236 Missing Key Allows for one decimal point, 981 9999 \O N -‘ II II II II II II \OU‘IJ: w N—e 99 \OOUWZLAJN—fi \O \OO‘szNJ \OO‘U1BwN—a 22.5 inches = Left blank Yes No Left blank Same house Within walking distance Within daily driving distance Over 100 miles Over 1000 miles Left blank Left blank Left blank Married Divorced Widowed Single Separated Other Left blank Married Divorced Widowed Single Separated Other Left blank Left blank White Black Hispanic American Indian Oriental Other Left blank Variable Family Income Occupation 052A 0528 053 059A Column Card Number of 59-60 61-62 13 15 Columns 237 Missing Key Values 99 O‘KfiBWN-J \OZWN-‘i \O wN—A \D ‘1 \O ANN—A under 5,000 5,000-10,000 10,000-15,000 15,000-20,000 20,000—25,000 25,000-30,000 30,000-35.000 35,ooo-uo,ooo over “0,000 Left blank At hospital Phone call Told in person Other Left blank New mother New father Both new parents Spouse Other person Saw or heard baby being born Cannot be determined Left blank Yes No Cannot be determined Left blank Yes No Cannot be determined Left blank Variable 0598 056 057 058 059 060A Column Card Number of 17-18 19 20 21 22 23 6 Columns 2 238 Missing Key Values 99 sozwm-a \OWNJ VDLAJNd \O Uni“)-a \O (DNONmth-J 1 day or less Within 1st week Within 1st month Within 2nd month Within 3rd month Within Ath month Within 5th month Within 6th month Between 6 and 9 months Between 9 and 12 months After 1 year Not applicable Left blank Yes No Other response Left blank Positive reaction Negative reaction Other reaction Left blank Positive reaction Neutral reaction Negative reaction Other reaction Left blank Yes No Did not have a preference = Left blank Daily 3-6 times/week 1-2 times/week 1-2 times/month Bimonthly Quarterly Other response Has not seen grandchild yet = Left blank Variable 0608 061 062A 062B Q63 069 Column Card Number of Columns 29-25 26 27 28 29 3O 6 2 239 9 Missing Key Values «is «Arc-a \ouaw-a so \OEWNd \OUWtUJNfl Daily 3-6 times/week 1-2 times/week 1-2 times/month Bimonthly Quarterly Twice/year Once/year Do not plan to see grand- hild 1st year Other Left blank Yes No Did not have expectations in regard to involvement Cannot be determined Left blank Yes No Other Left blank Positive changes Negative changes Both positive and negative changes Not applicable Left blank Yes No Other response Not applicable Left blank Positive Neutral Negative Mixed Other Left blank Variable 065A 0658 066 067 068 Column Card Number of 31 32 33 3“ 35 Columns 1 240 Missing Values 9 Key \OU‘EWN‘ \O B WN‘ \O 5' LAN—D an. \OU11‘: u: N Yes No No other grandparents = Cannot be determined Left blank Yes No No other grandparents = Cannot be determined = Left blank Yes No To some extent Not sure Other Left blank Subject lists a satisfying aspect of grandparenthood Subject does not list a satisfying aspect of grandparenthood Subject indicates uncertainty about the most satis- ying aspect Left blank Not being with grandchild often enough Worry about the grandchild = Particular infant behaviors There are none Other response Left blank Variable 069 070A 0708 071 Column Card Number of 36 37 38 39 Columns 1 241 Missing Values 9 Key Yes No Did not have any expectations Left blank \0 Cum-- I: Yes No Other Left blank \DLAJN—A II II II II ...n I - Weekly or more frequently = Every other week Monthly 2-u times/year 1 time/year Don't know Other response Not entitled to any contact Left blank N o mfiomzw u u n I: II n _; II Yes, subject offers advice 2 : No, subject does not offer advice 9 = Left blank Appendix E Prenatal and Postnatal Reliabilities Postnatal Reliabilities: Question 52A Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 52B Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 53 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Question 54A Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 5MB Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 56 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 57 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 58 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 59 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Question 60A Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder COED OOCD COED COO! 0003 now now CCU) COED 0003 21111 Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Open-Ended Questions .978 .978 1.00 .868 .928 .909 .978 1.00 .978 1.00 1.00 1.00 .936 09811 .952 .939 .897 .957 1.00 0696 .696 Question 608 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 61 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Question 62A Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 628 Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 63A Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 638 Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 65A Coder A and Coder Coder A and Coder Coder B and Coder Question 658 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 66 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 67 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder now now COO! 0013?) noun noon 00111 now now 0001 245 Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa .750 .805 .7fl5 .819 .89“ .778 .9116 .963 .91-11-1 .978 1.00 .978 .870 .877 .808 .784 .7u2 .790 .853 .931 .767 .799 .803 .686 .905 .88” .790 .728 .833 .795 Question 68 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 69 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 70A Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 708 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Question 71 Coder A and Coder A and Coder B and Those questions (prenatal questions: Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder Coder noon OOCU 0003 0003 COED Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa Kappa .937 .953 .922 .865 .828 .958 .867 .787 .878 .898 .860 .950 .969 .968 .937 53A, 538, 64, 65) for which the mean reliability between raters remained below revision were not categorized. 2N6 .7 after List of References REFERENCES Abraham, K. (1955). Some remarks on the role of grandparents in the psychology of neuroses. In HAL Abraham (Edd, Clinical papers and essays lg psychoanalysis. London: The Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1913). Albrecht, R. (1962). The role of older peOple in family rituals. In C. Tibbitts and W. Donahue (Eds.), Social and psychological aspects 3; aging: Aging around the world. New York: Columbia University Press. Babbie, E. R. (1973). Survey research methods. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing. Bacon, L. A. (197“). Early motherhood, accelerated role transition and social pathology. Social Forces, _5_22 333 " 31110 Beattie, W. M. (1975). Discussion. Gerontologist, 12, 39 - 140. Benedek, T. (1970). Parenthood during the life cycle. In E. J. Anthony and T. Benedek (Eds.), Parenthood: Its psychology and psychppathology. Boston: Little, Brown and Company. Blau, Z. S. (1973). Old age i_ a changing society. New York: Franklin Watts, Inc. Bordon, B. (1946). The role of the grandparents in children's behavior problems. Smith College Studies 13 Social Work, 11, 115 - 116. Borland, D. C. (1978). Research on middle age: An assessment. Gerontologist, 18, 379 - 386. Bortner, R. W. (1978/79). Notes on expected life history with introduction by Daniel J. Levinson. International Journal 9; Aging and Human Deve10pment, g, 291 - 29A. Boyd, R. R. (1969). The valued grandparent: A changing social role. In W. T. Donahue, J. L. Kornbluh and B. Power (Eds.), Living 1p the multigeneration family. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute of GerontElogy, The University of Michigan. 247 Brandt, C. (1968). Tips from an old pro grandmother. Home Life, 32, 26 — 28. Breen,l“ Z. (1960). The aging individual. In C. Tibbitts (EdJ, Handbook 2: social gerontology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Bryan-Logan, B. N. and Dancy, B. L. (19711). Unwed pregnant adolescents: Their mothers' dilemmas. Nurses Clinics pf North America, 9, 57 - 68. Cavan, R. S. (1962). Self and role in adjustment in old age. In A. M. Rose (Ed.), Human behavior and social process: gg interactionist approach. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Clavan, S. (1978). The impact of social class and social trends on the role of grandparent. Family Coordinator, 21, 351-358. Clavan, S. and Vatter, E. (1972). The affiliated family: .A device for integrating cum! and young. Gerontologist, 12, A07 - A12. Cohler,EL J.and Grunebaum,!L V.(1981). Mothers, grandmothers and daughters: Personality and child care lg three generations. New York: John Wiley and Sons. - Cottrell, F. (1960). The technological and societal basis of aging. In C. Tibbitts (Ed.), Handbook 2; social gerontology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Crase,IL R.and Hendrickson,!L (1968). Maternal grandmothers and mothers as perceived by pre-teen children. Journal 2:.Home Economics, 99, 181 - 185. Crawford, M. (1981). Not disengaged: Grandparents in literature and reality, an empirical study in role: satisfaction. American Sociological Review, 22, A99 - 519. Davis, R. H. (1977). Grandparenting: Varying themes in films. Gerontologist, 11, A5 - A7. Deutscher, I. (1962). Socialization for postparental life. In A. M. Rose (Ed.), Human behavior and social process: gg interactionist approach. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 2M8 Donahue, W., Orbach, H. L. and Pollak, O. (1960). Retirement: The emerging social pattern. In C. Tibbitts (Ed.), Handbook 3; social gerontology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Drevenstedt, J. (1976). Perceptions of onsets of young adulthood, middle, and old age. Journal 2: Gerontology, 31, 53 - 57. Duvall, E. M. (195“). In-laws pro and con: gg original study 2; interpersonal relations. New York: Association Press. Feldman, S. S., Biringen, Z. C. and Nash, S. C. (1981). Fluctuations of sex-related self-attributions as a function of stage of family life cycle. Develppmental Psychology, 11, 2A - 35. Ferenczi, S. (1927). The grandfather complex. In J. Hickman (Ed.), Further contributions 39 the theogy and technique 22 psychoanalysis. New York: Boni and Liveright. Fischer, L. R. (1983). Transition to grandmotherhood. The International Journal 2; Aging and Human DeveIOpment, lg, 67 - 78. Fried, E. G._and Stern, K. (19A8). The situation of the aged within the family. American Journal gf Orthopsychiatry, 18, 31 - 5D. Friedmann, E. A. (1960). The impact of aging on the social structure. In C. Tibbitts (Ed.), Handbook 2: social gerontology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Glick, P. C. (1955). The life cycle of the family. Marriage and Family Living, 11, 3 - 9. Glick, P. C. (1977). Updating the life cycle of the family. Journal 2: Marriage and the Famil , _3_9_, 5 -130 Greene, R. R. and Polivka, J. S. (1983, November). The meaning of grandparent's day cards: An analysis of visual images and implications. Paper presented at the meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, San Fransciso, CA. 2&9 Hader, M. (1965). The importance of grandparents in family life. Family Process, 1, 228 - 2A0. Hagestad, G.CL and Speicher, J.lu (1981). Grandparents and family influence: Views of three generations. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child DeveIOpment, Boston, MA. Hareven, T. K. (1977). Family time and historical time. Daedulus, 106, 57 - 70. Hartshorne, T. S. and Manaster, G. J. (1982). The relationship with grandparents: Contact, importance, role conception. International Journal 2: Aging Egg Human Develppment, 12, 233 - 2&5. Havighurst, R. J. (195“). Flexibility and the social roles of the retired. American Journal 21 Sociology, 22, 309 — 311. Hays, W. L. (1973). Statistics for the social sciences. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Hess, B. B. and Waring, J. M. (1978). Parent and child in later life: Rethinking the relationship. In R. M. Lerner and G. B. Spanier (Eds.), Child influence 2g marital and family interaction: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press. Jackson, D. W. (197"). Advanced aged adults' reflections on middle age. Gerontologist, 11, 255 - 257. Johnson,(L L. (1983). A cultural analysiscfi‘the grandmother. Research 2g Aging, 5, 597 - 567. Kahana, E. (1969, March). Grandparenting as a function of family structure. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Deve10pment. Santa Monica, CA. Kahana, E. and Coe, R. M. (1969). Perceptions of grandparenthood by community and institutionalized aged. Proceedings 9: the 77th Annual Convention 21 the American Psychological Association, 1, 735 - 736. Kahana, B. and Kahana, E. (1970a). Children's views of their grandparents: Influences of the kinship system. Gerontologist, 12, 39. 250 Kahana, B. and Kahana, E. (1970b). Grandparenthood from the perspective of the develOping grandchild. Develppmental Psychology, 3, 98 - 105. Kahana, B. and Kahana, E. (1980, April). Grandparents' attitude toward grandchildren as a function of marital-divorce status of their children. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Ortho-Psychiatric Association. Toronto, Canada. Kahana, E. and Kahana, B. (1971). Theoretical and research perspectives on grandparenthood. Agi g and Human Development, g, 261 - 268. Kornhaber, A. and Woodward, K. L. (1981). Grandparents/ grandchildren: The vital connection. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press. LaBarre, M. B., Jessner, L., and Ussery, L. (1960). The significance of grandmothers in the psychopathology of children. American Journal 2; Orthopsychiatpy, 39, 175 - 185. Lamb, M. E. (1975). Fathers: Forgotten contributors to child develOpment. Human DeveIOpment, 18, 2A5 - 266. Lee, D. M. (1982). Mothers of teenage parents: "Benefits or burdens?". Paper presented at the meeting of the National Council on Family Relations. Lee, D. M. and Colletta, N. D. (1983). Family support for adolescent mothers: The positive and negative impact. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit, MI. Lee, D. M. (198“, August). When grandparenting is off schedule: Roles, burdens and relationships. In B. Kahana (Chair), Grandparenthood: Implications for adult development and intergenerational relationships. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. Lopata, H. Z. (1973). Widowhood 1g,gg American city. Cambridge, MA: Schenckman Company. Matthews, 5.1L and Sprey,.L (198A). The impact of divorce on grandparenthood: An eXploratory study. Gerontologist, 25, A1 - A7. 251 Mead, M. (1997). What is happening to the American family? Journal 2: Social Casework, 22, 323- 330. Mead, M. (1966). What is the modern grandparent's role? Redbook, 22, 30. Mead, M. and Heyman, K. (1965). Family. New York: The MacMillan Company. Morgan, C. M. (1937). The attitudes and adjustments of recipients of old age assistance in upstate and metropolitan New York. Archives 2; Psychology, Number 21A, 1 - 131. Neugarten, B. L. (1968). The awareness of middle age. In B. L. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle ggg Egg aging: g reader 1g social psychology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Neugarten, B. L. and Datan, N. (1973). Sociological perspectives on the life cycle. In P. B. Baltes and K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Life-span develgpmental psychology: Personality and socialization. New York: Academic Press. Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., and Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms, age constraints, and adult socialization. American Journal 92 Sociology, 19, 710 - 717. Neugarten, B. L. and Weinstein, K. (196“). The changing American grandparent. Journal 22 Marriage and Family, 29,199 - 2011. Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. 0., Steinbrenner, K. and Bent, D. H. (1975).. SPSS: Statistical package for the social sciences (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw- Hill Book Company. Olsen, N. J. (1976). The role of grandmothers in Taiwanese family socialization. Journal 2: Marriage and the Family, 3g, 363 - 372. O'Rand, A. M. and Henretta, J. C. (1982). Women at middle age: Developmental transitions. Annals g; the American Academy 22 Political and Social Science, RED, 57 ~76A. Parsons, T. (19A3). The kinship system of the contemporary United States. American AnthrOpologist, XLV, 22 - 38. 252 Pihlblad, T. and Habenstein, R. W. (1965). Social factors in grandparent orientation of high school youth. In A. M. Rose and W. A. Peterson (Eds.), Older peOple and their social world. Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis Company. Poston, D. and Gotard, E. (1977). Trends in childlessness in the United States, 1910 - 1975. Social Biology, 211, 212 - 22a. Powers, E. and Brubaker, T. (1977). Social networks. In E. A. Powers, P. M. Keith and W. J. Goudy (Eds.), Later life transitions: Older males 1g rural America. (Sociology Report 1397. Ames: Iowa State University, Department of Sociology and Anthropology. Price-Bonham, S., Pittman, J. F., Jr., and Welch, C. 0. (III). (1981). The father role: An update. Infant Mental Health Journal, 2, 26A - 289. Rappaport, E. (1958). The grandparent syndrome. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 21, 518 - 537. Richardson, F. H. (196“). Grandparents and their families: 1 guide for three generations. New York: McKay. Riley, M. W., Foner, A., Hess, B. and Toby, M. L. (1969). Socialization for the middle and later years. In D. A. Goslin (Edd, Handbook 22 socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally and Company. Robertson, J. F. (1975). Interaction in three-generation families, parents as mediators: Toward a theoretical perspective. International Journal 2; Aging Egg Human Development, 2, 103 -110. Robertson, J. F. (1977). Grandmotherhood: A study of role conceptions. Journal 9: Marriage and the Robins, L. N. and Tomanec, M. (1962). Closeness to blood relatives outside the immediate family. Marriage and Family Living, 23, 3A0 - 3A6. Rollins,EL C.and Galligan, R. (1978). The develOping child and marital satisfaction of parents. In R. M. Lerner and G. B. Spanier (Eds.), Child influences 2g marital and fam11y interaction: 1 life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press. 253 Rosenmayer, L. and Kockeis, E. (1962). Family relations and social contacts of the aged in Vienna. In C. Tibbitts and W. Donahue (Eds), Social and psychological aspects 2: aging: Aging around the world. New York: Columbia University Press. Runyon, R. P. and Haber, A. (1972). Fundamentals g; behavioral statistics (2nd ed.). Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley. Shneidman, E. S. (1967). Sleep and self-destruction. In E. S. Shneidman (Ed.), Essays 1g self-destruction. New York: Science House. Smith, B. K. (1973). Aging 1g America. Boston: Beacon Press. Smith, E. W. (1975). The role of the grandmother in adolescent pregnancy and parenting. Journal 22 School Health, XLV, 278 - 283. Sprey, J. and Matthews, S. H. (1982). Contemporary grandparenthood: A systemic transition. Annals g; the American Academy 22 Political and Social Science, H63, 91 - 103. Staples,IL. and Smith,.L W. (1954). Attitudes of grandmothers and mothers toward child rearing practices. Child Development, 22, 91 - 97. Streib, G. F. and Thompson, W. E. (1960). The older person in a family context. In C. Tibbitts (Ed.), Handbook 22 Social Gerontology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, ““7 - “88. Sussman, M. B. (195“). Family continuity: Selective factors which affect relationships between families at generational levels. Marriage and Family Living, 19, 112 - 120. Talmon-Garber, Y. (1962). Aging in collective settlements in Israel. In C. Tibbitts and W. Donahue (Eds.), Social and psychological aspects 2; aging: Aging around the world. New York: Columbia University Press. Thomas, J.Iu and Datan,bL (1983, August). Change and diversity in grandparenting experience. Presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Anaheim, CA. 25” Tibbitts, C. (1960). Origin, sc0pe and fields of social gerontology. In C. Tibbitts (Edd, Handbook 22 social gerontology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Tibbitts, C. (1965). Middle-aged and older people in American society. Selected Papers 2g Aging, 1, St. Louis, MO: Gerontological Society, Projects Division. Thompson,1L E.and Streib,CL F. (1962). Meaningful activity in a family context. In C. Tibbitts and W. Donahue (Edsd, Social and psychological a§pects g; aging: Aging around the world. New York: Columbia University Press. Townsend, P. (1957). The family life 3: old people. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Treas, J. and Bengtson, V. L. (1982). The demography of mid- and late-life transitions. Annals 22 Egg American Academy 2: Political and Social Science, 333, 11 - 21. Troll, L. (1971). The family of later life: A decade review. Journal 22 Marriage and Family, 33, 263 - 290. Updegraff, S. G. (1968). Changing role of the grandmother. Journal 2: Home Economics, 99, 177 - 180. Vollmer, H. (1937). The grandmother: A problem in childrearing. American Journal 2: Orthopgychiatry, 1, 378 - 382. VonHentig, H. (1935/36). The sociological function of the grandmother. Social Forces, 23, 389 - 392. Wahl, A. (1968). Who are the elderly? - An overview. Selected Papers 2g Aging, 1. St. Louis, Missouri: Gerontological Society, Projects Division. Waller, W. and Hill, R. (1951). The family: 1 gynamic integpretation. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. 255 Winch,1L F. (1963). The modern family(2nd ed). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Wood, V. and Robertson, J. F. (1976). The significance of grandparenthood. In J. F. Gubrium (Ed.), Time, roles, and self in old ag . New York: Human Sciences Press. 256