MSU RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LJBRARJES remove this checkout from —,——. your record. FINES wil] be charged if book is returned after the date stamped beIow. 11 ‘ DEPRESSION AND PERCEIVED MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS BY Paula C. McNitt A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1985 ©1986 PAULA CHANLEY MCNITT All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT DEPRESSION AND PERCEIVED MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS BY Paula C. McNitt This study presents a family-systems conceptualization of clinical depression and hypothesizes that depression is a homeostatic mechanism in the marriages of depressed men and women. Depressive symptoms preserve the marital status quo and the relationship rules that govern interaction between the depressed client and his/her spouse. The specific pre- dictions tested were that depressed psychotherapy clients would report less autonomy, higher levels of hostile auton- omy, dominance and submission, less affectionate inter- change, greater hostility, and more severely impaired communication in their marriages than would either psychi- atrically normal adults or other psychotherapy clients. A group of 16 outpatient psychotherapy client volun- teers who received a diagnosis of primary unipolar depres- sion (Depressed Clients) was compared with a group of 16 psychotherapy clients who received some other diagnosis and with 16 psychiatrically normal adults. Each participant rated his/her marriage during the past three months on Series A and B of the Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (Benjamin, 1979), a modified version of the Love Scale (Swensen and Gilner, 1968) and the Marital Communication Inventory (Bienvenu, 1978). The results of the study provide partial support for its hypotheses. The Depressed Clients reported less overall autonomy and friendly autonomy and more hostile autonomy, dominance and submission, more hostility, and more impaired communication than did the Normals. Contrary to prediction, the Depressed Clients reported no less affection in their marriages than did either the Normals or Nondepressed Clients. Only on the self-ratings of overall autonomy, friendly autonomy, hostile autonomy, and submission were the differences between Clients groups statistically signifi- cant. In rating their spouses' behavior the Depressed Clients differed from the Normals, but not from the Non- depressed Clients. DEDICATION For my daughters: Megan Elizabeth and Katherine Anne ii . Oil-u .- UOc‘ . . Ola but. an. (ll CAM ‘ V‘. (’n' o f ACKNOWLEDGMENTS There are literally dozens of people to whom I owe thanks for their assistance and support in the conduct of this study and the preparation of this dissertation. First and foremost I must thank the participants, who must remain anonymous. Their time, effort, and trust could hardly be repaid by my modest "Thank you” payments. Next, I must thank the clinical staff of the cooperating mental health centers. A dozen or more therapists proved to be a most interesting and colleagial referral source. The members of my dissertation committee, Professors Dozier Thornton, Bertram Karon, Robert Zucker, and Elaine Donelson are as fine mentors as a student of psychology could seek. And there are those "on the home front" to be thanked. My husband, Andrew McNitt, provided the moral and financial support to see me through this long project. Catherine Smith, my day-care provider, took such good care of Megan and Kate, that I felt quite free to write this dissertation. Terri Monnett has made the final preparation of this manuscript seem almost effortless. To all of these people I say "Thank you" for helping me to achieve this goal. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O The Significance of the Marital Relationship . . . . . . . . . Systems Theory and Psychopathology. . . Depression as an Interpersonal Process. . . . . . . . . . . The Depressed Patient and His/Her Family . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . METHODS O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 subjects I O D O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Client Participants. . . . . . . . . Inclusion Criteria for the Depressed Clients Group. . . . . . . . . Inclusion Criteria for the Nondepressed Clients Group. . . . . . . . . . . Psychiatrically Normal Participants . . Inclusion Criteria for Psychiatrically Normal Participants. . . . . . . . Matching Participant Groups . . . . . Criteria for Matching Couples. . . Procedure . . . . . . . . . . Briefing Clinicians and Establishing Diagnostic Agreement. . . . . . . Contacting, Interviewing and Testing Client Participants . . . . . . . Orientation Protocol. . . . . . . . . . Instructions for Participants (A) . . . Recruiting and Testing Psychiatrically Normal Participants. . . . . . Instructions for Participants (B) . . Feedback. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . Validity Studies. . . . . . . . . . . Organization and Analyses of SASB Data The Love Scale. . . . . . . . . Love Scale Scores and the Marital Relationship . . . . . . . . . A Modified Version of the Love Scale iv Page PI r'J CHAPTER The Marital Communication Dependent Measures. . Predictions . . . . . 3. RESULTS . . . . . . . 4. DISCUSSION. . . . . . Appendices A. TARGET GROUPS . . . . B. GUIDELINES FOR BRIEFING PARTICIPANTS. . . . CLIENT Inventory . . . DIAGNOSING PRIMARY UNIPOLAR DEPRESSION. . DEPARTMENTAL RESEARCH CONSENT FORM. . . C D. SAMPLE CASE HISTORIES E F . INFORMATION ABOUT: THE MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS OF PERSONS EXPERIENCING DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY . G. GUIDELINES FOR INTERVIEWING THE H. SUMMARY OF DIAGNOSTIC DATA. I. GENERAL INFORMATION SHEET . BECK'S DEPRESSION INVENTORY CLIENTS . A MARITAL COMMUNICATION INVENTORY . SASB QUESTIONNAIRES . J K L. SCALE OF FEELINGS AND BEHAVIOR OF M N . INTRODUCTION TO PARTICIPANT COUPLES . . O. SURVEY OF PROBLEMS. . References 0 O O O O O O O O A STUDY INVESTIGATING Page 102 104 104 110 145 175 176 177 179 184 185 186 187 191 192 193 199 212 221 222 223 16. 17. 18. LIST OF TABLES Referral Sources of Client Participants Characteristics--Depressed Clients. Characteristics--Nondepressed Clients Comparison of Groups on the Beck Depression Inventory. Referral Sources of Psychiatrically Normal Participants . Matching Couples Across Groups. Comparison of Couples/Groups on Age, Length of Marriage, and Social Class . . . Operationalization of Dependent Measures. Comparison of Comparison of Comparison of Comparison of Comparison of Comparison of Comparison of Affection . Comparison of Affection . Comparison of Comparison of MCI Scores - C Groups on Interdependence . Groups on Friendly Autonomy Groups on Groups on Groups on Groups on Groups on Groups on Groups on Groups on 1 O O 0 vi Dominance Submissiveness. Hostile Autonomy. Affiliation Expression of Expression of Hostility . Communication- Page 44 51 53 57 59 64 66 105 111 112 113 114 115 120 121 122 123 126 ~-.. 0! ‘UO 1 x. -a ‘6'. Table Page 19. Correlations - Severity of Depression and Autonomy, Affiliation, and commication O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 127 20. Correlation Matrix - 18 Dependent Measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129 21. Summary of Step-wise Discriminant Function Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132 22. Comparison of Standardized Dis- crimination Coefficients and Structure Coefficients. . . . . . . . . . . . 136 23. Classification of Cases . . . . . . . . . . . . 140 24. Assignment of Cases to Groups According to Discriminant Scores. . . . . . . . . . . . 141 vii mu. RI. LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Map of SASB Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 2. Map of SASB Octants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 viii .C 'E b on” . us :n "—L ‘44 on. CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCT ION This study examines several aspects of the marital relationships of depressed men and women. Specifically, it focuses on autonomy vs. dependency, expression of affection and of anger, and quality of communication in these mar- riages. The study's theoretical underpinnings are to be found in the family systems theorizing of Jackson (1968), Haley (1963a), and Satir (1967). A family-systems con- ceptualization of psychopathology defines a disorder as an expression of disturbance not merely in the identified patient's intrapsychic functioning, but also in his/her family system. Psychiatric symptoms are homeostatic mechan- isms insofar as they preserve the family members' roles and relationship rules. The fundamental hypothesis of this study is that clinical depression is a homeostatic mechanism in the marital relationship of depressed men and women. Depressive symptoms are congruent with these couples' general interaction processes. A family-systems conceptualization differs importantly from genetic and biochemical (Akiskal & McKinney, 1975), psychoanalytic (Gaylin, 1968), cognitive (Beck, 1972), operant-behavioral (Lewinsohn, 1974), and learned .-n So)!“ 3335 ‘IH’ ‘anH. baa... 9» .h A v +5 ~ V. . . a m“ . C t c s . .3 mm- aw I - A~5 ‘4‘ .: ~V9H§ Quad 5.» 9|. .nu . v. . . a i C s, e «G ~D 2“ c M. u. . u . . |.R M \ \Q h D. 2 helplessness (Seligman, 1974) theories of clinical depres- sion. These otherwise diverse theories agree that the prime cause of depressive pathology is to be found within the individual. A person responds to life's stresses and dis- appointments with depressive symptoms because there is some- thing wrong with him/her: his/her body chemistry is askew, s/he has directed hostility against him/herself rather than against a disappointing love object, s/he is excessively dependent on others, s/he systematically misinterprets events in an egocentric and negativistic fashion, s/he lacks the social skills necessary’ to {elicit the reinforcement which would maintain his/her self-esteem and self-confi- dence. Generally, these theories assume that the depressed patient's current social environment is, at most, a precipi- tating or secondary factor in the emergence of his/her disorder. The depressed person is a relatively closed system, impervious to his/her sane and rewarding environ- ment. S/he is, however, unable to acquire the available support and gratification s/he so desperately needs (Coyne, 1976a). A family-systems conceptualization, though, assumes that an individual's depression is an index of disturbance in a relationship system in which his/her intrapsychic and physiological processes are but sub-systems. This concep- tualization does not repudiate the significant contributions of biochemical, psychodynamic, cognitive and social learning theories of depression. Rather, the family systems ar- v.- A.-. It . G Pg v. .au Q. .C .G .3 .u.. 0" cu . UV . s t. E. is. .2 to s . he . .. .. x 5—» \oc 3 orientation offers an additional conceptual level from which to examine the phenomena of clinical depression. It is, perhaps, an oddity' of contemporary psycho- pathology research that only a few scattered investigators [the Hogans in the United States (Hogan & Hogan, 1975), Hinchliffe and her colleagues in England (Hinchliffe, Hooper & Roberts, 1978), and Heins in Australia (Heins, 1978)] have adopted a family systems approach to the study of depres- sion. While this theoretical orientation has been widely heralded as a nmjor advance in clinical research, the vast majority of depression research has remained focussed on intra-individual variables. This focus may be due in part to the behavior of the depressed patient him/herself (Hinchliffe et al., 1978). The depressed person tends to be self-absorbed. and. egocentric in, his/her' view' of his/her predicament. The very language in which s/he expresses and describes his/her feelings and problems diverts his/her, and at times the researcher's, attention from the depression as an interpersonal process. A shift in focus from the identi- fied patient to his/her family system can be difficult for patient, therapist, and researcher. Yet there is ample justification for a systems ori- ented study of the depressed patient's family relationships. First, this orientation promises further understanding of depression. because it Ihas enhanced. our understanding of other psychiatric disorders, notably schizophrenia. Secondly, clinicians, both published (Deykin, Jacobson, V. ‘ - 5C.“ 0 \f‘ (9\ RA- EVA. An. ~..- . R- ‘V 1‘. V ‘. It." ch ’ 1 ) 4 Klerman & Solomon, 1966; Feldman, 1976; Forrest, 1969; Hogan & Hogan, 1975; Jacobson & Klerman, 1966; Rashkis, 1968; Rubinstein & Timmons, 1978) and unpublished (this author) have concluded that a comprehensive assessment and treatment of the depressed patient must address itself to his/her family and to the function his/her depression serves for that family. Klerman and his colleagues (Deykin et al., 1966; Jacobson & Klerman, 1966) observed that depression in some middle-aged women both stemmed from and served to mask conflicts over changes in relationships with their grown children. Rashkis (1968) concluded that the emergence of an adolescent child's identity crisis can elicit similar unresolved conflicts and consequent depression in his/her parent. Forrest (1969) describes a pattern of shared underlying pathology in the marriages of several of her severely depressed patients. Several behavioral (Lewinsohn & Shaffer, 1971; McLean, Ogston & Grauer, 1973) and cognitive-behavioral (Rush, Shaw & Khatami, 1980) therapists have reported that their treatment efforts ‘were greatly enhanced by enlisting the active participation of a depressed patient's spouse. Ideally, one would examine all of the depressed person's important family relationships. Practical and empirical-theoretical considerations, however, have limited this study to an investigation of the marital relationships of currently married depressed outpatients. - Ff. ooh 5" v. .Lq oaoc ' A. s . ‘ Utou‘ ASA Vc¢5 0" “I c O A: Vs 5 The Significance of the Marital Relationship Epidemiological and life history research suggest that the marital relationship plays a central role in the onset and persistence of depressive disorders. With most psychi- atric disorders it is the unmarried who are at higher risk. The reverse is true for depression; it is the married who are at higher risk (Hinchliffe et al., 1978). Overall's (1971) study indicates that. depressive symptomatology is characteristic of psychiatric patients who have been married once. This group includes currently married patients as well as divorced patients who have not remarried. Those patients who had never married were more likely to present schizophrenic symptoms, and those who had married more than once were unlikely to present high levels of any one cluster of psychiatric symptoms. From this Overall (1971) argues that depression is related to dependency relationships in less than adequate social settings. Several lines of research have demonstrated an associ- ation between marital unhappiness and clinical depression. Conflict and change in the depressed patient's marriage have often been cited as stressful events preceding the onset of a depressive episode. Paykel and colleagues (Paykel, Meyers, Dienelt, Klerman, Lindenthal & Pepper, 1969) found that among 185 psychiatric patients receiving a diagnosis of primary depression, an increase in arguments ‘with one's spouse was the most frequently mentioned source of stress p O .312 C0" ‘n‘a La. 5‘ 5o. 5‘ VFI' 6 occurring during the six months prior to symptom onset. The number of depressed patients reporting increased marital conflict was statistically higher than the number of matched normal controls reporting this problem. In this study marital separation. was the second. most frequently cited stressful event. Leff, Roatch and Bunney (1970) in their intensive long-term. study’ of 40 severely' depressed inpatients noted that 19 patients reported a change in their marital relationships during the six months prior to their depressions. The emergence of depressive symptoms hardly eliminates marital dissatisfaction and discord. Depressed persons often report continuing stress and ‘unhappiness in their marriages. In a study of the general population Ilfield (1977) found that of all social stressors, it is marriage stressors which correlate highest with depressive symptoms. Coleman and Miller's (1975) study of 154 couples who requested help at an outpatient mental health clinic yielded a significant correlation between self and clinicians' rat- ings of depression and ratings of marital maladjustment. The correlation was significant only for the husbands, although the wives were more depressed and more often the identified patient. An extensive investigation of the social adjustment of 40 depressed women (Weissman & Paykel, 1974) revealed high levels of friction and hostility between these patients and their husbands which persisted months after symptomatic recovery. 7 Of the 76 depressed women participating in another Yale study (Rounsaville, Weissman, Prusoff & Heiceg-Baron, 1979) over half reported serious disputes with their husbands, and of this half only one-fourth reported improve- ment in their marriages during eight months of maintenance treatment. The women who reported no marital improvement were significantly more symptomatic at treatment's end than were all of the other participants. All of the women in this study had initially shown marked symptomatic improve- ment following four to six weeks of anti-depressant medica- tion. At the beginning of subsequent maintenance treatment there was no relationship between symptom severity and marital discord. The patterns of marital discord were remarkably stable at 48 months follow-up. Few of the women who initially reported no marital disputes later reported such problems. Most of the women who had initially com- plained of disputes with their husbands did so 48 months later. The between group differences on severity of depres- sive symptoms was evident at 20 months, but not at 48 months follow-up (Rounsaville, Prusoff & Weissman, 1980). These researchers (Rounsaville et al., 1979) suggest that the marital conflicts of depressed women are chronic sources and exacerbators of depressive episodes rather than transient by-products of the latter. The evidence suggesting that the marital relationship often figures significantly in the development of depression prompts the further, rather difficult question of the 9- be. a: ‘ I o A§b an .. a v. Y. .84 «4 a: I. Q. ‘ to n . Fifi Q 0%-. “n\ ukfi ‘5 u‘l‘ \ ~§ s: .2 I. a. h s a.‘ 5‘. 8 specific nature of the relationship between the depressed person's mood and behavior and the dynamics of his/her marriage. To argue that depression is simply one partner's reaction to a disappointing or conflict ridden marriage leaves much unexplained, for the question remains as to why that partner becomes depressed rather than develops other psychiatric symptoms, seeks consolation in an extra-marital liaison, becomes abusive, or quits the marriage. It must be noted. as well that the co-occurrence of depression and marital dysfunction is not universal. Not all of the depressed women in the Yale studies (Rounsaville et al., 1979, 1980) reported marital disputes. In Coleman and Miller's (1975) study the correlation between depression and marital maladjustment proved statistically significant only for the husbands. Perhaps an answer to this quandry can be found in a somewhat different conceptualization of the rela- tion. .between. interpersonal. relations and. psychiatric symptoms. This conceptualization posits depression as the out- come of basic, underlying marital processes and rules when these are subjected to stress, rather than as one partner's idiosyncratic response to marital dysfunction. The under- lying marital roles and rules which give rise to depression are the same ones which may give rise to overt conflict and unhappiness. A marital relationship which is not obviously unhappy or conflicted may nonetheless be depressogenic. Hinchliffe and her colleagues (Hinchliffe et al., 1978) AHA .Au 5:; are At 5.. 9 argue similarly that the depression prone marriage. may be superficially adequate and supportive. On the surface there are few, if any, signs of conflict or poor communication. At a deeper level, though, there may be a far more disturbed level of experience which is quite negatively toned. Family systems theory provides a vocabulary with which to elaborate and elucidate the relationship between marital processes and the emergence of clinical depression. Systems Theory and Psychopathology General systems theory (von Bertalanffy, 1974) pro- vides the conceptual framework for a family-systems study of psychopathology. The theory has as its basis the concept of the self-regulating, goal directed biological organism. A system is a complex of parts and processes in mutual inter- action. Causal connections within the system are circular rather than linear. Any component acting within the system will ultimately be acted on by other components in response to its original action. This process is called a feedback loop. Organisms are self-regulating and maintain a state of internal dynamic equilibrium, or homeostasis. Systems are error activated and self-correcting. If any component or subsystem diverges from its appropriate path or level of activity, other components will act on it so as to bring it back into line. Such error correcting processes are referred to as negative, or deviation reducing, feedback a: 1'. A f5 at F a .JJ . a A: an“ .. e A» v. a . a 3 s S A. e e u“ e Ob ‘HI n.v . a“ “L a. a» ‘ ‘3. ”“4 \.¢ an Pt Did ..u 6. V‘ n .n AU an .~§ .hu 6. I» . In. ' 9" “a. UC ~ ‘1‘ by A! t . ht \ s 5 8 .flve WM hK» 1. y 10 loops. Implicit in the concept of homeostasis is that organisms resist fundamental, system incongruent change. The application of systems theory to the study of psychopathology has utilized family homeostasis (Jackson, 1957) as its pivotal concept. The family is a self- correcting system in which all members act to maintain its steady state. Homeostasis entails a balance in the intra— familial relationships (Satir, 1967). If any member trans- gresses either the behavioral limits imposed by his/her role in the family or the family's rules for interaction with outsiders, other family members will act so as to rescind or reduce that transgression. Family' members behave as if governed by rules (Jackson, 1965). Family rules usually remain unstated, but can be inferred from the members' habitual interaction patterns and their responses to devia- tion from these. All family members govern each other's behavior, although they may not be aware that they do so (Haley, 1963a). Governing includes far more than straight- forward domination; the processes of interpersonal control entail a complex and subtle array of overt and covert, active and passive, concrete and symbolic, rewards, threats, and punishments. Psychiatric disorders are homeostatic mechanisms. Symptoms are not simply senseless, maladaptive behaviors, but rather the system's response to internal and external threats to its equilibrium. As painful and distressing as these symptoms may be for the patient and his/her relatives, 01-; boob 7.65 r“ .. ‘L“ .0 o: 11 they may be necessary for the family's survival. The family needs to have one of its members serve as "identified patient.” This seemingly counterintuitive conclusion has been drawn from two classes of clinical observation. First, families frequently attempt, whether consciously or uncon- sciously, to sabotage the identified patient's treatment and recovery (Kohl, 1962; Satir, 1967). Perhaps the most startling instance of such sabotage is the unwitting abetting of their husbands' drinking observed in some wives of alcoholics. Ewing (Ewing, Long & Wenzel, 1961) noted that some of these women's sense of strength and effective- ness was contingent on the husband's remaining dependent and incapacitated by his drinking. A husband's sobriety was so threatening as to prompt some wives to ”accidentally“ serve their otherwise sober husbands alcoholic beverages. Secondly, in some families, members alternate in playing the role of identified patient. Many clinicians have observed the emergence of psychiatric symptoms in another family member consequent to the identified patient's improvement. Kohl (1962) found that of the 39 spouses of hospitalized psychiatric patients, five resumed excessive drinking, and 21 became depressed following the first signs of the patient's symptomatic recovery. Jackson (1957) noted several instances of symptom exchange, including the case of a husband's sexual impotence which emerged consequent to his wife's improvement in her treatment for frigidity. These I y . r ) 12 cases are cited as examples, not as proof of the universal- ity, of symptom exchange within families. Psychiatric symptoms are homeostatic in two senses. First, symptoms can maintain the family as a unit when it might otherwise disintegrate under stress. Symptoms pre- serve unstable and unrewarding relationships within the family, especially the marital bond. Children, it seems, possess an uncanny proclivity to develop emotional, behavioral, and academic problems which deflect their parents' attention and energy away from their own marital discord (Satir, 1967). In a sense, a sick child can preserve a bad marriage. Should the child's difficulties be resolved, his/her parents would again be faced with disap- pointments and conflicts in their marriage. Similarly, a husband's or wife's anxiety, depression, agoraphobia, alcoholism, etc., can save a couple from confronting their relationship problems and thereby from risking a complete disruption of their marriage (Haley, 1963b). Husbands and wives have been known to collude in the myth that they would be quite happy together if only he or she were not plagued by this or that symptom. The symptom, they will protest, is strictly the individual's problem, and has nothing to do with their marital relationship. Psychiatric symptoms are homeostatic in that they are congruent with the family's relationship and communication rules. For example, Haley's (1959) model of the family communication and relationship rules of the schizophrenic 13 patient makes sense out of his/her psychotic behavior. Com- munications within the schizophrenic's family are such that family members constantly negate or disqualify their own and each other's statements. Such communications entail an incongruence between the message, or content, level and the meta-message level, which indicates how the message content is to be understood. Because all communications define the relationship between the sender and his/her audience (as well as convey content) incongruous messages deny or confuse this relationship. Consequently, the schizophrenic experi- ences extreme difficulty discerning his/her relationships to other and thus his/her own identity as well. Moreover, the schizophrenic's family invokes prohibitions on his/her attachments both in and outside of the family. Such pro- hibitions become unbearable as he/she confronts his/her own maturational needs and the larger society's demands that he/she individuate from his/her family. Psychotic behavior is the schi20phrenic's way of simultaneously infringing and not infringing these prohibitions. The patient withdraws into his/her private reality, but remains dependent on his/ her family's care and support. He/she denies his/her identity and his/her relationship to others, but does so in a manner consistent with the only communication rules he/she has ever learned. That is, his/her delusions and his/her "word salad" statements are messages which are incongruent at all levels. In short, he/she has reduced his/her family's mode of interpersonal transaction to an absurdity. 14 Homeostasis as a first principle of family process is both limited and pessimistic (Speer, 1970). It cannot account for the normal, healthy growth of the individual or of the family as a relationship system. Secondly, the principle implies the imminent capacity for psychiatric dis- order in any family. Redefining the family as an open, but relatively stable system can, however, enhance understanding of both normal and disturbed families. The family is governed by both deviation reducing and deviation amplifying feedback mechanisms (Speer, 1970; Wender, 1968). As such it is both self-regulating and. open to change and growth. Families whose internal organization is complex and flex- ible, and whose processes are limited by a minimum of rigid constraints change and grow in response to the demands of both its members' maturational needs and its changing environment. This capacity for adaptation and growth is what differentiates healthy from pathogenic families. Only sick, symptom-generating families are in homeostasis (Speer, 1970). Satir (1971) has similarly argued that the rules of dysfunctional families interfere with the growth and development of their members. Given such rules, these families are bound to respond to the challenges and stresses imposed by maturational and environmental forces with symptoms. Such families can maintain their status quo only if at least one member becomes dysfunctional. The fundamental hypothesis of this study is that clinical depression, as opposed to periods of normal grief, 15 or of reality based discouragement, is a homeostatic mechan- ism in each of the two senses discussed. First, depressive symptomatology can maintain the marital unit when it might otherwise disintegrate. Secondly, depressive symptoms are congruent with the couple's special mode of interaction. Depression, insofar as it is a passive, dependent, whiny, reproachful, manipulative, and indirect plea for care and attention, is consistent with a couple's relationship rules. That depression, or any other psychiatric disorder, serves to keep a marriage or family intact cannot be directly observed, but can only be inferred. Similarly, relationship rules can only be inferred from observed patterns of behavior. A couple's interaction pattern can, though, be observed or reported on directly, and thus it is to the interactions reported by depressed men and women that this study addresses itself. Specific hypotheses are based on the case study and research literature on the marital and family treatment of depression. Depression as an Interpersonal Process The interpersonal characteristics of depression can be discussed from two viewpoints. One approach focuses on the depressed patient's interpersonal needs and style: on his/ her dependency, hostility and uncommunicativeness. The psychoanalytic tradition has emphasized the depressive's excessive and unresolved dependency needs (Arieti & Bemporad, 1978; Chodoff, 1970, 1974). The depression-prone :41 .4 n. 4 c 4.. an. e .u.. e v. at in we 1 pk .s‘ «k ~\U .C .4 I. a“. .u C a z 3. .L ... «e no ..~ It ~ . 16 person requires an inordinate amount of attention and praise from others in order to maintain his/her precarious self- esteem and self-confidence. S/he lacks that stable sense of self-worth which enables most adults to weather life's frustrations and disappointments with relative equanimity. The depressive's dependency needs become most obvious during symptom episodes. Beck (1972, p. 32) observes that the depressed patient expresses a craving for help that exceeds even his/her perceived need of help. The 40 depressed women of Weissman and Paykel's (1974) study reported that they relied on their husbands to help with household chores and child care and make decisions for them while they were depressed. Once their depressive symptoms lifted, however, they no longer relied on their husbands so extensively. Cohen et a1. (1954) described the manic depressive patient as maintaining one or a few highly dependent relationships. His/her need to be cared for and relieved of adult responsi- bility dominates all of his/her interpersonal transactions during depressive episodes. During symptom-free periods, the manic-depressive maintains an easy, though superficial, social adjustment. Even during his/her healthy periods s/he operates from the premise that life's rewards are to be won via ingratiation and manipulation of powerful others, rather than to be earned by one's own imperfect skill and effort. The manic depressive patient fears abandonment above all, and thus avoids any competition or conflict with significant 17 others. In short, there are two aspects of the depression- prone person's dependency needs: the reliance on the good opinion of others to maintain his/her sense of worth and the need to be cared for and absolved of responsibility. This latter aspect apparently comes to the fore during clinical episodes. The relationship between depression and hostility has been extensively discussed and researched. Abraham (1911) was one of the first psychoanalytic theorists to note the depressive's ambivalence and repressed hostility. Freud (1917) postulated an aetiological link between depression and introjected hostility. He noted that the depressed patient's self-denigration and self-blame are devoid of the humility one :might expect. of a ‘truly self—effacing and repentant person. From this, Freud inferred that the depressive's apparently self-directed anger has, in fact, a different target. This target is a lost or frustrating love object. whom. the depressive has introjected rather than relinquished. Freud's insight introduced the maxim widely held among clinicians that depression is associated with a maximum of self-directed hostility and a minimum of other directed hostility. Other clinicians point to hostility as a prominent feature of depression, but question the aetiological link Jbetween introjected hostility and depression. The research- ers from the Washington School of Psychiatry (Cohen et al., 1954) conceptualized the manic-depressive's hostility as a 18 product of his frustrated attempts at manipulating others into meeting all of his/her narcissistic needs. Hostility is a by-product, rather than a cause, of the depressive process. Chodoff (1970) comments that the impact on others of the depressive's behavior belies its unconscious hostile intent. That contact with a depressed patient can leave one feeling tense, vexed, guilty and depressed is attested to by clinical (Cohen et al., 1954) and laboratory (Coyne, 1976b) observations. Bonime (1960) may be the least sentimental of clinicians in his assessment. of the depressed patient's angry, sadistic manipulativeness. He contends that depres- sive symptoms entail two pathological goals: satisfaction of’ manipulating others to provide care and concern and enjoyment of the tyranny of dependency in the exploitation of others. Those who try to alleviate the depressive's misery usually find themselves ”walking on egg shells" from fear of further upsetting the patient. More often than not, the would-be helper ends up feeling impotent in the face of the depressive's stubborn misery. The patient's covert goal, Bonime (1960) contends, is to hurt and defy his/her significant others. The studies designed to systematically confirm clini- cal observations and intuitions of the depressed patient's overt and covert hostility have yielded varied results. Wessman, Ricks and Tyl's (1960) study of normal college women found that periods of normal low mood were associated with an increase of extrapunitive rather than AU Ab Vi. .v a.» 19 intropunitive responses to frustration. These psycho- analytically oriented researchers suggest that it is precisely the response to frustration which differentiates normal lOW' mood from pathological depression, and that pathological depression is associated with increased intro- punitiveness. A mmlti-trait, multi-method study (Zuckerman et al., 1967) of anxiety, depression and hostility yielded mixed results. Anxious and depressed psychiatric patients reported significantly' more .hostility' on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (Buss & Durkee, 1956) than did normal controls; however, these groups did not differ on inter- viewers' ratings or Multiple Affect Adjective Check List (MAACL) (Zuckerman & Lubin, 1965) self-ratings of hostility. The correlation between MAACL ratings of depression and Buss-Durkee hostility scores was significant for the normal subjects only. Gershon, Cromer and Klerman (1968) examined levels of "hostility-in" and "hostility-out" in the free association content of six depressed female inpatients. "Hostility-in" correlated positively with ratings of severity of depression for all six women. For only two of the women did "hostil- ity-out'I correlate significantly (and positively) with severity of depression. These two 'women. were rated as significantly lower on depressive affect, though not on overall symptom severity, than the other four. These two k , the ' I e 31 .I‘. For; _ o.‘ id L... a; 4.x, 5- 20 were also the only two described as presenting hysterical personality feature. The 190 depressed patients in Friedman's (1970) study reported significantly less verbal hostility and signifi— cantly more resentment on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (Buss & Durkee, 1956) than did the 98 normal control subjects. The depressed patients reported no greater internalization and somaticization of anger than did the normal subjects. Clinical improvement was associated with a significant decrease in all aspects of hostility. Friedman remarks that the patients' decrease in verbal hostility is curious as one would have expected a regression to the norm, and thus an increase in verbal hostility with clinical improvement. He speculates that the depressed person usually inhibits overt expression of anger, but this defense breaks down during depressive episodes. Friedman (1970) cites his previous finding that depressed patients were much less likely than nondepressed subjects to agree that sometimes it is quite right to be angry. He concludes that the inability to freely verbalize anger is an integral aspect of the predisposition to depression. Weissman and Paykel (1974) assessed the amount. of friction, defined as overt fighting, depressed women experienced with their children, husbands, relatives, friends, and co-workers. Compared to psychiatrically normal women, these patients reported significantly more friction during' both the symptomatic period and the twelve-month "fl (1) 58‘. G‘s ,- U. ”I 4'. L.‘ raw 0‘: e s I 21 recovery period. Friction correlated with the intimacy of the relationship. The women argued most with their children and husbands and least with their friends and co—workers. weissman and Paykel (1974) conclude that the expression of hostility varies according to the depressed woman's social setting. The amount of anger she manifests or alludes to during an assessment interview may be a very poor indicator of the kind and amount of friction she is experiencing at home. These authors also comment that their clinical obser- vations revealed no impairment in the depressed woman's overt expression of hostility, but noticeable impairment in her direct communication of her needs, wishes, and feelings. In a methodologically sophisticated study, Schless et a1. (1974) were able to divide 37 depressed inpatients into two groups: internalizers and externalizers of hostility. Among internalizers severity of depression correlated posi- tively with degree of internalization. Externalization of hostility was associated with hysterical personality features and resentment. Schless et a1. (1974) suggest that hostility serves as a defense against experiencing depres- sive affect. In severe cases of depression this defense has broken down. On a semantic differential measure severely depressed patients described their own anger as impotent but other people's anger as quite potent. Fava and his colleagues (Fava, Kellner, Munari, Pavan & Pesarin, 1982) compared a sample of 40 outpatients who were experiencing a first depressive episode sufficiently Fug 'Vu h... ~Mc «Q a: u be 22 severe to warrant treatment with antidepressant medication with a sample of psychiatrically normal controls. The former group scored significantly higher on hostility as assessed by the Kellner Symptom Questionnaire (Kellner, 1981). Among the depressed patients, the correlation between ratings of hostility and of severity of depressive symptoms proved statistically reliable only for those patients who had suffered no losses or exits from the social field during the several months prior to symptom onset. Presumably those patients who had lost a significant other had no (one to blame for their unhappiness, whereas those patients who had not lost a significant other were both angry and depressed as a result of ongoing interpersonal conflicts (Fava et al., 1982). Although results of these studies provide no single unequivocal statement about the relationship between hostility and depression, they do suggest that the depressed person is less able to express his anger verbally, in a straightforward manner. Anger expression may be a defense against the experience of depressive affect. Depressed patients whose anger is expressed in the form of resentment and hysterical complaints experience less severe feelings of depression. Patients who turn their anger against them- selves experience more severe depressive feelings. A third feature of the depressed patient's inter- personal transactions is his/her impaired communication (Coyne, 1976; Spiegel, 1965; Stuart, 1967). Any therapist 23 who has attempted to engage a severely depressed patient in talking therapy can testify to the extreme difficulty of the task (Spiegel, 1965). Impaired communication is hardly limited to the consultation room; rather it appears to be a prominent feature of the depressed patient's family rela- tionships. McLean, Ogston and Grauer (1973) observed that their depressed patients' intimate relationships were marked by a coercive style of communication which precluded effective problem solving. These patients and their spouses proffered each other predominantly negative feedback when discussing interpersonal issues. Moreover, they were often unaware of the impact such feedback had on their partners. Their case histories also indicated that this negative mode of communi- cation was chronic and not limited to one partner's symptomatic periods. Weissman and Paykel's (1974) study of depressed women revealed an abnormal degree of reticence in their relation- ships with their husbands, children, other relatives, neighbors, and co-workers. This reticence was not limited to the depressive episode, but persisted during the twelve- month symptom-free follow-up period. The. content of the depressives' messages was also constricted. For the most part, depressed patients talked about their symptoms and their general unhappiness. Wasli's (1977) study of 16 hospitalized depressed women and their husbands revealed that these couples' 24 conversations were brief, infrequent, and most often centered on the wife's feelings. These depressed women and their husbands reported difficulty perceiving, understanding and discussing each other‘s needs, discussing problems, and expressing disagreement without losing their tempers. The patients often complained that their husbands did not mean what they said. On a standardized instrument, the Marital Communication Inventory (Bienvenu, 1978), the couples reported significantly impaired communication. Hinchliffe, Hooper, and Roberts (1978) examined the patterns of communication between 20 depressed inpatients and their husbands and wives. Their discussions of hypo- thetical interpersonal and family problems videotaped during the acute depressive period were compared with discussions taped at symptomatic recovery, discussions between the depressed patient and a stranger, and discussions between psychiatrically normal surgical patients and their spouses. The depressed patient couples were generally more expressive than were the surgical control couples. This differenoe was most pronounced for negative expressiveness in the messages of the depressed men at the acute sympto- matic stage. At recovery these patients had reduced negative expressiveness to the same levels as the surgical controls. The depressed women and their husbands made no such shift. Their conversations were highly negatively toned at both the symptomatic and recovery periods. The depressed patients and their spouses were more formal in 25 their conversational style, given to more "turn taking." In discussing solutions to problems the depressed patients and their spouses were more egocentric and less objective than were the psychiatrically normal couples. The depressed couples' conversations had a negative uneven flow, often disrupted by pauses and negative emotional outbursts. In contrast, the conversations of the surgical couples were leavened by laughter and other positive tension releasors. During the acute symptomatic period the depressed patients attempted to exert greater control by means of interruptions and direct eye gaze. Yet these deviant patterns of communi- cation were not apparent in the depressed patients' conver- sations with strangers. This suggests that the depressed patient's impaired communication does not necessarily generalize to all of his/her relationships. Hautzinger, Linden, and Hoffman (1982) compared the communication. patterns of two groups of’ couples seeking marital therapy. In the first group one partner was diagnosed as depressed; in the second group neither partner was so diagnosed. The depressed partner couples expressed more dysphoric mood, talked more about their well being, and offered each other more help than did the other couples. The nondepressed spouses of the depressed clients, compared to both spouses in the second group, expressed fewer nega- tive and more positive self-evaluations, evaluated their spouses less positively, seldom agreed with them and offered them more help. Such help was ambivalent as it was usually 26 accompanied by negative evaluations of the depressed part- ner. The communication in the distressed couples without a depressed partner proved to be more positive, reciprocal and supportive. A. second. approach to the interpersonal aspects of clinical depression defines depressive symptoms as inter- personal behaviors. Bonime (1960) emphasizes that depres- sion is an active, though manipulative, way of relating to others. Although the depressed person is often withdrawn and uncommunicative, s/he nonetheless broadcasts his/her misery to all around him/her. Depressive symptoms are messages demanding action (McPartland & Hornstra, 1964). These are pleas for others to provide support and meaning. Symptoms are, however, oblique and strident pleas, and thus usually fail to elicit the desired response. Each failure only intensifies the depressed person's pleas. His/her messages become ever more strident, reproachful and unanswerable. The patient's audience, which may at one time have been specifiable, becomes increasingly diffuse. Should this depressive drift continue unchecked, the patient may abandon all hope of having his/her pleas met and retreat into a psychotic state (McPartland & Hornstra, 1964). The depressed patient's uncommunicativeness is itself a message. The depressed person uses behavior rather than words to convey his/her meaning (Stuart, 1967). His/her communication is paradoxical insofar as it is a plea that denies it is a plea. Because all relationships are defined 27 through communication processes, the depressive's symptom messages can be construed as attempts at revising his/her relationships. The essence of the depressive's message is "you must treat me differently, but, I am not telling you to do so, because I do not control my depressive behavior" (Stuart, 1967). The depressed person can dominate his/her entire social field, yet remain convinced of his/her utter helplessness. Because it is an active way of relating to others, depression both influences and is influenced by its inter- personal context. Several authors have commented on how the depressed individual's social environment elicits and rein- forces his/her symptoms. Operant behavior theorists, notably Lewinsohn and his colleagues (Lewinsohn, 1974; Libet & Lewinsohn, 1973) contend that depression is a response to a low rate of response contingent positive reinforcement. A real or imagined loss of a significant source of positive reinforcement can account for both the depressive's low rate of adaptive behavior and his/her depressive affect. The sympathy which depressive symptoms normally elicit inevit- ably reinforces these maladaptive behaviors. Lewinsohn's group argues that the depressed individual's lack of social skills is a critical antecedent to his/her loss of social reinforcers. Libet and Lewinsohn's (1973) study of depressed college students' participation in small group discussions revealed that these students addressed t y. s V ‘ .e... Eh... slit AV. ‘09:. “s ‘9‘». ML we .fi‘ ‘ 28 themselves to fewer fellow group members and offered fewer positive responses to them than did non-depressed students. Lewinsohn's group has also investigated the role played by the patient's home environment in the emergence and maintenance of clinical depression. Lewinsohn and Shaffer's (1971) home observations of the marital inter- action of several of their depressed clients revealed that these clients received more negative and less positive reinforcement from their spouses than they in turn offered. In several cases the interaction, whether positive or nega- tive, between client and spouse was minimal. Liberman and Raskin (1971) applied a functional analysis of depressive symptoms to their family-based treatment of depression. They noted that family members habitually focus their attention on a patient's dysphoria, guilt, and fatigue, and overlook the patient's adaptive behaviors. Thus they advised families to ignore the patient's depressive com- plaints and attend to and praise all of his/her adaptive, constructive behaviors. This strategy proved effective in alleviating depressive symptoms. Coyne (1976a) presents a sophisticated model of the interaction between depressed persons and their significant others. The depressed person engages others, but in such a way as to lose their support. Depressive symptoms are a call for help; they demand restoration of a valued source of reinforcement and reassurance of the depressed person's role and value in his/her social space. Typically these messages 29 of need, pain and helplessness elicit another's attempt to answer these demands directly and literally. Yet the depressed person cannot decide whether the proffered reassurance is a statement of genuine concern or merely a response to manipulation. S/he therefore intensifies his/ her complaints and demands for assurances and validations of his/her worth. This prolonged intensification of symptoms is both frustrating and guilt inducing for the would-be helper. S/he may overtly deny his/her anger and resentment at being imposed on, but s/he is likely to express these feelings nonverbally. The ambiguity of the helper's messages reinforces the depressed patient's belief that s/he is not truly loved and cared about. This belief further intensifies his/her symptom pleas for love and understand- ing. Thus emerges a pattern of mutual manipulation in which each participant denies that the manipulation is in fact occurring. Eventually, more would-be helpers drop out of the depressive's social field. As his/her social space contracts, the depressed patient's pleas grow increasingly ambiguous and diffuse. Coyne .(1976b) tested. the hypothesis that. depressed persons induce hostility and depression in those with whom they interact and thereby court rejection. In his study, college women conducted telephone conversations with depressed clinic clients, nondepressed clinic clients, or normal women. The students who conversed with the depressed women subsequently felt more depressed, anxious and hostile, 30 described their conversation partners less favorably, and expressed less interest in further contact with these women than did the students who conversed with the other two groups. These results provide experimental confirmation of the common clinical observation (Cohen et al., 1954; Spiegel, 1965) that the depressed client can be a most trying, irritating, and fatiguing person with whom to interact. The question which now presents itself is how and why the depressed person's close friends and relatives con- tribute to an apparently difficult and unrewarding relation- ship to the extent that they do. The literature on the family treatment of depression suggests answers to this question. The Depressed Patient and His/Her Family Several authors (Hogan & Hogan, 1975; Rubinstein & Timmons, 1978) emphasize that the exaggerated dependency characteristic of the depressed patient entails a two-person system and that this dependency relationship is the core feature of the families of these patients. Hogan and Hogan (1975) note that these families promote and prolong the infantile polarity of powerlessness to act directly on the environment, on the one hand, and omnipotence to get others to meet all of one's needs, on the other. Powerlessness is manifest not only in inadequate task performance, but also in the inability to perceive and express needs and feelings directly. Infantile omnipotence involves the expectation () ,. . Cut I. RI‘ ydo‘l ‘ .' 54“ me: u s .. a. ‘4ou. 31 that one will be viewed as special and rewarded without one's effort, the tendency to uuhimize efforts not crowned by perfect success, and the belief that mistakes cannot be corrected. Such dependency entails a two-person relation- ship in that it includes an omnipotent other who will spontaneously recognize and meet all of the dependent one's needs. The omnipotent other is supposed to decipher all of one's covert and inarticulate requests. This dependency relationship between mother and infant is a normal but brief developmental stage. Most mothers and children progress beyond it as the child acquires those skills which enable him/her to negotiate the world on his/ her own. Some parents, however, out of their need to be indispensable, perfect, all-giving, parents, subtly and not so subtly, discourage and punish the child's self-assertion and independence. In such families the child learns that to assert his/her wants and needs and to make his/her own deci- sions is to impugn his parents' caretaking ability. S/he is taught that one makes requests of strangers, not of loved ones, for loved ones are supposed to know one's needs with- out being told. Consequently, the child learns to devalue any praise or rewards or affection not offered spontane- ously. The prohibition against self-assertion also places severe constraints on the child's experiencing and express- ing his/her strong feelings, especially his/her anger. Expression of anger is felt to be quite dangerous because it belies the belief that others in the family will meet one's 32 needs ‘without. being asked. Anger thereby threatens the family's symbiotic bonds. Suppression of anger not only reinforces the child's feelings of powerlessness, but also instills the belief that. his/her emotions are extremely destructive forces. The family interaction processes which discourage autonomy and self-assertion can be discerned in both the family of origin and the current immediate family of the depressed patient. This is no accident: in adult life, men and women often find partners with whom they can replicate the relationship patterns of their childhood (Hogan & Hogan, 1975; Jackson, 1957; Rubinstein & Timmons, 1978). That the depressed adult patient maintains the position of infantile polarity of powerlessness and omnipotence implies the s/he is abetted by a significant other who assumes the role of omnipotent parent or rescuer. This is precisely the observation made by clinicians (Rubinstein & Tummons, 1978) who argue that every case of depression involves a depressive dyad consisting of the patient and his/her caretaker. The caretaker’ may' be a parent, child, spouse, friend, or therapist, but typically is the patient's spouse. Partners in such dyads have been known to exchange roles as well. The underlying dynamic in the relationship is anger. In cases of severe depression, the patient is enraged at not having all of his/her needs met, and the caretaker is enraged by his/her failure to meet the patient's extreme dependency needs. This anger must be transformed though, because it threatens the relationship. 33 The patient's anger is transformed into helplessness and depression; the caretaker's anger is transformed into even more solicitous care. Rubinstein and Timmons note a similar dyadic pattern in cases of neurotic depressive disorders. In these cases the hostility is more overt and dissatisfactions are verbalized. Some couples are caught in a continual, though perhaps unconscious, struggle over who will be the one cared for by the other. Pathological elements in the marital relationship are evident even during symptom-free periods. These elements provide the basis for episodes of depression. Rubinstein and Timmons (1978) characterize these marriages as disallow- ing personal growth or autonomy. Neither partner can feel free as this would threaten the other's security. Each partner monitors and supervises the other's behavior. The relationship is over-close and over-warm, but the partners are unable to share responsibility for problem solving or to collaborate in professional or leisure interest. In this sort of relationship, warmth and closeness have become forms of bribery and coercion. Feldman (1976) comments similarly on the patient- rescuer marital pattern in depression. He describes a circular interaction. pattern. in.*which the husband's and wife's complementary cognitive structures trigger repetitive reciprocal stimulation and reinforcement. Operationally, this means that the nondepressed partner's overprotective- ness elicits and reinforces the depressed partner's sense of 34 helplessness, inadequacy and resentment. The depressed partner's symptoms, in turn, elicit and reinforce the caretaker's overprotectiveness. The nondepressed partner unwittingly undermines the depressed one's autonomy and self-esteem. Each partner may resent his/her role in a relationship which s/he nonetheless needs. The depressed spouse needs to be cared for and reassured of his/her wealth; the caretaker needs to be needed. The caretaker may initially welcome his/her depressed spouse's recovery, but may also respond negatively to any changes which challenge the basic marital bargain. Feldman (1976) cites the case of a husband who was at first delighted by his wife's recovery from depression, but who grew extremely jealous and resent- ful of her increasing independence and involvement in her new job. The caretaker's need to be needed. may ‘well. be a transformation of his/her own unmet dependency needs. Tabachnik's (1961) study of the interpersonal relationships formed by suicide attemptors (who are likely to be depressed) found that these typically include a unique relationship with a single significant other. This signifi- cant other assumes responsibility for meeting the suicide attemptor's inordinate dependency needs, even to the point of being imposed on. Tabachnik speculates that in doing this the rescuer is covertly and vicariously meeting his/her own dependency needs, which s/he otherwise must defend against. The suicide-rescuer relationship is not, however, 35 unambivalent. The suicide attemptor may need to be punished so as to alleviate his/her guilt over his/her inordinate needs and his/her rage at not having these met. From either his/her anger at being imposed on or his/her envy of the attemptor's dependency, the rescuer may eventually punish the suicide attemptor. This punishment may assume the form of criticism, anger, neglect or insensitivity. In short, this is a highly symbiotic relationship, but one punctuated by ambivalence. Forrest (1969) in her long-term intensive treatment of five cases of severe depression, observed a somewhat differ- ent pattern of underlying pathology in her patients' mar- riages. These couples interact so as to perpetuate their mutual feeling of grief and deprivation. Each partner main- tains an inordinate .need. for help and. support. and. each appeals to the other's grandiosity in attempting to persuade him/her to assume the role of the all-good provider. They battle constantly for the dependent role. Neither partner can, of course, meet the other's needs, the caretaker role notwithstanding. The depressed partner neither trusts nor values the care and concern his/her spouse does offer as these were gotten by manipulation. The relationship is, to say the least, emotionally impoverished. Yet the partners collude in denying and suppressing their mutual anger and frustration. A picture of the depressed patient's marital relation- ship emerges. This is a tentative composite, which does not C)" u: U) rt 0 36 purport to mirror every married patient's relationship with his/her spouse. This is a marital relationship constrained by excessive interdependency. This interdependency becomes most apparent when one partner becomes depressed. His/her depressive symptoms are covert and overt demands for atten- tion and care. On an overt level the well partner accepts responsibility for attending to, caring for and reassuring the depressed spouse. His/her acceptance of the caretaking role is a function of the guilt and discomfort depressive symptoms elicit in others and of the vicarious gratification and security to be derived from meeting the other's dependency needs. On a covert, as well as overt, level the caretaker expresses his/her anger and resentment at being imposed upon. The depressed partner also harbors anger and resentment at his/her dependent position. This anger finds expression in depressive symptoms, as well as in overt friction. Despite these ambivalences, the relationship is basically shaped by both husband's and wife's mutual needs to care and be cared for by each other. Although the rela- tionship is over-close, it is not necessarily emotionally rewarding for either partner. Their exchange of affection, care, and support is limited by the superordinate constraint of their bargain of mutual dependency. Neither partner's needs for care and affection can ultimately be met for such security and satisfaction would liberate the partners from their symbiotic bond. As a rule, the depressed person and spouse cope poorly with the disagreements and conflicts. AH .uu s a 37 They rarely confront their differences and disappointments constructively. In cases of severe depression, it appears, patient and spouse collude in denying their anger and dis- appointment. In cases of less severe depression, the marital partners expend their energy in bitter arguments which probably focus on issues irrelevant to their real conflict. The communication process in these marriages is impaired. These couples do not express and explore their wants, needs, and feelings; rather they expect each other to know these without being told. These relationship patterns are most pronounced during periods of one partner's depres- sion, but are also discernible during symptom-free periods as well. Hypotheses The hypotheses tested in this study are extrapolations from a diverse, and not wholly consistent, body of case study and research literature on the interpersonal, and specifically marital, relationships of depressed persons. The basic hypothesis, of this study, which can be tested only indirectly, is that clinical depression serves a homeostatic function in the .marital relationship of the depressed psychotherapy client. What can be demonstrated directly is that the interaction patterns of depressed persons and their husbands and wives are congruent with, that is, these promote and reinforce, depressive sympto- lnatology. The following’ hypotheses predict reliable 38 differences between depressed client couples and psychi- atrically normal couples and between depressed client couples and couples in which one partner presents neurotic or personality problems other than primary depression. The central hypothesis is: Hypothesis 1: The marital relationship of depressed clients is marked by a higher degree of interdepen- dence than that of either psychiatrically normal adults or other psychotherapy clients. a. Depressed clients and their spouses relate to each other as autonomous adults less than do normal husbands and wives and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. b. Depressed clients and their spouses manifest more dominance, control, submis- sion and deference than do normal husbands and. wives, and other psycho- therapy clients and their spouses. The depressed client's relationship is not, however, unambivalent, in that both partners, and especially the "well" spouse, periodically' withdraw' in. anger from. this over-close relationship. Consequently, the second hypothesis is: Hypothesis 2: Depressed clients and their spouses invoke +> .n- " 1 (N ' 1 39 and assume hostile autonomy more often than do psychiatrically normal husbands and wives and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. The third hypothesis pertains to affect exchange in the marriage. Hypothesis 3: The marital relationship of the depressed client is less emotionally rewarding for partners than is that of psychiatrically normal adults and of other psychotherapy clients. The assumption here is that an emotionally rewarding rela- tionship is characterized by a predominance of affection over hostility. Thus: a. Depressed clients and their spouses express less affection and exchange fewer verbal and non-verbal loving' behaviors than do normal husbands and wives, and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. The relationship of the depressed client and his/her partner is marked by a greater degree of hostility than are the relationships of normal couples, and of other patients and their partners. However, the relationship between 'l" .-h ’5 RP. ‘1 4 .1‘ ‘3‘ .c PH. 40 depression and overt expression of hostility is problematic. While Paykel and Weissman's (1974) study disclosed an abnormal. amount. of friction. between. depressed. women and their husbands, Forrest (1969) reports that severely depressed patients and their partners collude in denying their mutual hostility. Most of the women in Weissman and Paykel's study were diagnosed as neurotically depressed. This suggests that hostility expression varies with intensity of the depression. It may be that the relation- ship is an inverted U. Couples in which one partner is mildly to moderately depressed experience more overt hostility than do normal couples. Couples in which one partner is severely depressed collude in suppressing any expression of anger. As this study focuses on outpatients whose depressions are in the mild to moderate range, the following hypothesis is proposed: b. Depressed clients and their spouses express more overt hostility and friction than do either normal husbands and wives or other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. The fourth. hypothesis dovetails ‘with. the ;preceding ones . Hypothesis 4: The communication processes of depressed client couples are more impaired than those 41 of normal couples and nondepressed client couples. Depressive symptoms are a form of indirect and ambiguous communication, and as such are congruent with the generally impaired communication between depressed men and women and their spouses. Indeed, symptom complaints may be the only channel left open in a communication system in which neither partner can either acknowledge or accurately interpret changing needs and stressors in their' marital relationship. These hypotheses have been tested only with respect to the patient's marital relationship during a three-month period which includes the early stage of the depressive episode. Such limited data can neither specify which comes first, the abnormal interaction patterns or the depressive symptoms, nor demonstrate a causal relationship between these two phenomena. All that confirmation of these hypotheses can estab- lish is the co-occurrence of depressive symptomatology with certain abnormal features of marital interaction which are proposed as) congruent with the symptoms themselves. Yet this evidence can be construed as preliminary systematic support for a family-systems conceptualization of depres- sion. CHAPTER 2 METHODS Subjects The 48 men and women who were selected for inclusion in this study were married (or in one instance stably cohabiting) residents of downstate Illinois. At the time of their participation all were living with their spouses and only one had been separated from his spouse for longer than a week during the past three months. The participants were all Caucasian, aged 20 to 62. Thirty-eight participants were women; ten were men. Forty reported a religious affiliation: 27 with a Protestant denomination, 11 with the Roman Catholic Church, and one each with the Mormon and Seventh Day Adventist Churches. The 48 participants were comprised of three groups of 16. The target group of interest consisted of 16 depressed outpatients. This group, to be referred to as the Depressed Clients group, was compared with two control groups--the Nondepressed Clients and the Psychiatrically Nermal Group. The 32 volunteers in the two clinical groups were clients either of one of eight community mental health or counseling centers or of one of two registered psychologists in private practice in Charleston, Illinois. The 16 normal controls 42 43 were recruited from the general adult population of Coles and Edgar Counties, Illinois. Client Participants The 32 client participants were selected from a pool of 50 clients who had given their therapists permission to refer them to the principal investigator. Table 1 presents the nine referral sources and the number of included par- ticipants each referred to the study. With the exception of Decatur Mental Health Center, these agencies serve rural and small town communities. Decatur Mental Health Center serves a more urban, industrialized county of 250,000. Eighteen client volunteers were ultimately dropped from the study because they failed to meet the inclusion criteria for either the Depressed Clients or Nondepressed Clients groups. At the .beginning' of data collection. the principal investigator had stipulated that a client would be included in the study only if his or her wife or husband participated as well. This requirement soon proved to be a major obstacle to conducting the study. Too often the spouse (and usually this was the husband) was either unwilling or unable to attend interview and questionnaire sessions. The spouse's participation was then made optional. Fifteen spouses participated in the study, almost all of whom either were or had been in conjoint therapy or were in some way actively cooperating with the identified patient's treat- ment. The data collected from these spouses had not, to (D D 9‘ 44 Table 1 Referral Sources of Client Participants Source Number Decatur Mental Health Center, 12 Decatur, Illinois Coles County Mental Health Center, 6 Mattoon, Illinois Psychologists in Private Practice, 4 Charleston, Illinois ‘Vermillion.County Mental Health Center, 3 Danville, Illinois Human Resources Center, 2 Paris, Illinois ‘V.A~ Medical Center, Mental Health Clinic 2 Danville, Illinois Counseling Center, Lakeland College 1 Mattoon, Illinois .Eastern Illinois university Counseling 1 Center, Charleston, Illinois Franklin-Williamson County Family 1 Counseling and Mental Health Services, Herrin, Illinois a: in 45 however, been included in the findings of this study because a statistical comparison of the two groups of spouse par- ticipants was not warranted.- The samples of the Depressed Clients and Nondepressed Clients spouses were too small and too dissimilar to be subjected to such analysis. Only five spouses in the Depressed Clients group participated in the study and they could not be matched to the ten other spouse participants on the relevant social and economic background variables. Each client participant included in the study was assessed as belonging in one of two diagnostic groups. Sixteen outpatients in the Depressed Clients group had received a diagnosis of primary unipolar depression. The Nondepressed Clients group consisted of 16 outpatients who had received a diagnosis other than primary depression or alcoholism. The inclusion criteria for each group are outlined below . Inclusion Criteria for the Depressed Client Group A participant is to be included in the Depressed Client group if and only if s/he meets criteria 1 through 3: 1. Receives a diagnosis of primary unipolar depres- sion according to the following criteria (A and B): A. A diagnosis of primary unipolar depression agreed on by the client's therapist and the 46 principal investigator. Diagnostic guidelines follow 'Diagnostic Criteria for Use in Psychi- atric Research' (Woodruff, Goodwin & Guze, 1974, pp. 199-209). For a diagnosis of depression, 1 through 4 are required. 1. Dysphoric mood characterized by symptoms such as the following: depressed, sad, blue, despondent, hopeless, "down in the dumps,” irritable, fearful, worried, or discouraged. 2. At least five of the following are required for "definite" depression; four are required for ”probable” depression. (1) Poor appetite or weight loss (positive if two pounds a week or ten pounds or more a year when not dieting.) (2) Sleep dif- ficulty (including insomnia or hyper- somnia). (3) Loss of energy, e.g., fatigability, tiredness. (4) Agitation or retardation. (5) Loss of interest in usual activities, or decrease in sexual drive. (6) Feelings of self-reproach or guilt (either may be delusional). (7) Complaints of or actually diminished ability to think or concentrate, such as slow thinking or maxed-up thoughts. (8) 47 Recurrent thoughts of death or suicide, including thoughts of wishing to be dead. 3. A psychiatric illness lasting at least one month with no pre-existing psychiatric conditions such as schizophrenia, anxiety neurosis, phobic neurosis, obsessive compulsive neurosis, hysteria, alcoholism, drug dependency, antisocial personality, homosexuality and other sexual deviations, mental retardation, or organic brain syndrome. (Patients with life-threatening or incapacitating medical illness preced- ing and paralleling the depression do not receive the diagnosis of primary depres- sion.) 4. No evidence of prior manic episodes. B. Scores 18 or higher on the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, 1972). This score was chosen as it was the mean for a group of inpatients and outpatients who were diagnosed as udldly depressed (Beck, 1972, p. 196). 2. Has not received electroshock therapy during the past six months. 3. Is not actively psychotic. Two amendments were :made in requirement. 3 of the "Diagnostic Criteria for Use in Psychiatric Research." 48 First, depressive symptoms need be present for only three weeks. Secondly, depressive symptoms need only predominate over rather than antedate other psychiatric symptoms (except for the case of schizophrenia). The rationale for this second amendment is that it was often impossible to ascer- tain whether and which other symptoms antedated a client's depression. A client's own recollection of the development of his/her emotional difficulties was often incomplete and vague. Also a diagnosis of ”probable depression" would suffice for a client's inclusion in the Depressed Clients group. Inclusion Criteria for the Nondepressed Clients Group A participant is to be included in the Nondepressed Client group if and only if s/he meets criteria 1 through 6: 1. Receives one of the following diagnoses as agreed upon by the client's therapist and the principal investigator: anxiety disorder-panic; generalized anxiety disorder, phobic disorder, obsessive- compulsive disorder, dissociative disorder, psychosexual disorder, personality disorder, borderline personality organization, schizophrenia in remission, adjustment disorder. 2. Does not receive one of the following diagnoses: primary unipolar depression, bipolar depression, 49 alcoholism or alcohol abuse, organic brain syn- drome, mental retardation. 3. Is not in treatment for a life-threatening ill- ness. 4. Has not received electroshock therapy in the past six months. 5. Is not actively psychotic. 6. Score$13 or below on the Beck Depression Inven- tory. This cut-off score was chosen because it would ensure that any client in this group would report at least two fewer depressive symptoms than any participant in the Depressed Clients group. In practice, diagnostic decisions were made by the principal investigator with the assistance of the referring clinician. The principal investigator had originally expected the referring therapist to provide a precise diagnosis for each client. This expectation proved overly ambitious. In many instances the referring clinician provided useful, and on occasion detailed, information about his client's problems, but bypassed a formal diagnosis according to the criteria provided. There appear to have been two reasons for this. First, the referring therapist often forgot to refer to the specific diagnostic criteria for the study, and relied instead on his/her own definitions of depression, anxiety disorders, personality disorders, etc. It is worth noting that clinical experience and 50 sophistication of training varied considerably among referring clinicians. Secondly, several clients presented symptom pictures sufficiently mixed as to make differential diagnoses rather difficult. In all but three cases the principal investigator's decision accorded with the referring therapist's diagnostic impressions. In these instances the referring clinician's diagnostic information was so limited and imprecise as to allow the principal investigator no choice other than to rely on her interview data in formulating a diagnosis. When the principal investigator had interviewed 40 client volunteers she reviewed their case histories, interview data and Beck Depression Inventory scores with her dissertation advisor, an experienced clinical psychologist. Together they decided which diagnostically dubious cases were to be excluded from the data analysis. Following this review the principal investigator interviewed additional client volunteers. Tables 2 and 3 present brief descriptions of each participant in the Depressed Clients and Nondepressed Clients groups, respectively. As can be inferred from these tables, the two groups are differentiable, but not wholly dissimilar. The clinical pictures of the Depressed Clients, although dominated by more numerous and more severe depres- sive symptoms, also included a range of other symptoms. One-third of these clients reported phobic, obsessive or psychosomatic symptoms. Two women in this group appeared Sl .moocfiucoo canon. ooowuumouo sewu00aooz Haswosq o mm ocean unwon mo pooh :memmumoo 5m m m unmounauso 6:02 NH mm unu :oammmuaoo «m m a Hfluoaaoz muofi>ocoo acowuomuso .ocoxcoua v ov o>wmanoeoo .mfinocmouomc newmmouooo hm m o anomawmw ocoz .m.uoNfiafisocoua m ow .mxoouuo xuowxnc :onmouooo mm m m Indus. monogauuoma ocoz Hw>o~m cm hm .oxomuuo xuowxcd scammouooo ov h v moaocoocou acowuoouoo ocoz 0 am u0w>ocoo o>wm~5oeou scammouooo on 2 m anewuoouso onwaauoauuHE< v mu III cofimmouooo mm m N meoanouo HoofimoHououoouczu acofiuoouao ocoz vN mm .mEoHnOuo vaudeouocoamm sawoooumoo mm 2 H aomuocuocoamm scauoo«ooz oxooz ouoom wousuoom ducacwao mqoocooao oo< xom ommu \ucoauooua acouusu acoEuooue Hon unaududcmfim wonuo quHm acouuau oucowao,oomuoumooulmowunauouoouoco N «Home 52 ocoz genomoo m w~ III: scammoudoo Nm m oH coda mason Hocoodo louaaouwomoz ouocoouoo mo auoumwc .uooHOuwo a acowuoouoo Eswcufiq oma Hm maggo20muoo deadwoouom :oMmmouooo we m ma omsoo Husxum auoumwc ooonpawao .mxomuum ucoaumaoao 6:02 «N am suaaonaufiuuouowcua :oommoumoo eN a «a ucowuoouso ocoz m an woeouuo ooMOAsm unooom sodomouooo mm 2 Ma scan uudfiouuo Ionaaouadwoz moaowsm mo auoumw: a ucowuoouso III o" o~ .mucwoaoeoo OquEom cofimmouooo om m NH dunno Hosxom ooocoawco mo auouuac .uooHOwwo ocoz awesomc «m hm >uwad:0muoo ocwauoouom scammouooo mm m ma Edwao> ocoouuauso .Hficuuuoa Ga aw u--- :owmmouamo mm m 0H ace» (mafiamuwomo: a unmauaauso convocam Ga on u--- :oammuuama Nm a a adouocuocoxnm noduoowoox oxooz ouoom mousuooh Hmowcaau afloocmowo 00¢ xom owou \ucoEumoua ucouuau acoeuooue mom acoowuwcmum nocuo uOHum acouusu ..o.ucoo. ~ canoe 53 .moocwucoo cacao. unmounauso coda mowuasofiu cowouMEou louwaouwomom ocoz om n Imwo amuwuofi ucouuao cw decoucQONAsom av : m mousuoow ucowuoouso Ho:0auoeo ooxwe cofiu muoeouuo oowuasm ocm cue: newuooou (unaauummmoz coveoma Ga n smaaozooaa do scone“: unusuaafloa em a c acoauoouso ouocoouoo «oesooaamomhc coho Eswcqu muoeouuo mowowom .o>wmmouooOIuw:oE nonwaouflowo: .~«>mHm NN o oddwuase mo auoumam .oacoucQONAcom mm m m udfiduuo ooaofiom oco cofimmouomo no wuoumaz .maouoE>m owoosm Am. amouOmwo ucowumouso ocoz ma v .oeouoe>u o>dmmomoo xuwHMCOuuom mm m N ouamoo Hosxom macawoouooo oouaoaccw uo huoumwc .muaouu .:o«uocsumao unawuoouoo ocoz o nu o>fimasoeoolo>ammomoo Hooxomocummm an m H acoEuooue :Owuuofiooz axooz ouoom mousuoom Hmowcaau mamocmoao om: xom onto uOAud ucouusu usafiuooua Hon acoowuwcoam uocuo . acouuso avocado oouooumoocozulmOwumwuouuouoco m manna 54 .mozcwucoo manna. anewuoeo ooxwe .cOauooou 0:02 6:02 n ~H u--- unusunsflca «N 2 Ha acoaummuso cacao :o«u :oHumoumoo uOHuo Iluoououwo (audaouwomo= uoHoEom hm o .mowocoocou Owoocm auowxcd mm m 0H uoouo nucaoaoeoo Imwo auaaoCOm ocoz cwocomc co m cauoEOo .neemmouooo sumo o>wnasoeoo Av S a uoouomwo auoaxcm ucowumduso .do:o«uoeo :OMu wowocoocou oowaIuoouOowo nouwuouaomoz 0:02 5 Ma Hoofloazmu:0wmmouooo acoeumsno< no 2 m monouoou Moccauoeo oox«Enc0wuooou ucwaomauao 8:02 an n u--- unoEUuafloa mm m a oooe mucuxco cue: :oHuooou ocoz ocoz oH m u--- unusumsfioa mm m o ucOEumoua scauoOMomx mxoo3 ouoom mousuMOh HoOwCMHU owoocmoao 00¢ xom omoo quum acouusu unoEuooua Hon unscauacmwm nocuo acouuso ..o.u:oo. n manna 55 no: .uouomwumo>:« .oomocoowo mcwuowawcoo odowuasa concouocw mouooou Hoowocwuo oco unwoouocu unannouou ocu coon how xuocoso o ooucomouo mwmocoowo m.:oeos mesh 4 meouoE>o meouoaam Hchwuoeo ooXHE o>wmmouooo IcoHuooou ocoz ocoz m 5 came one ucooom ocosumsfloa mm a G3 acofluoouso coho xuoaxcm cow: .COMm uoouomwo Imuwamufiomoz 0:02 on N umouooo odes oaoOHMom auwaocOmuod mm m ma moucuoou encoduoeo oostluoouo mcoz «:02 a a n--- -mun ucoeomsnoa mN a «a muoEmuum oaumwmmwouoc ucowumouso mowoaom acoo unmouowwo Amocoomoaooo. Immaooo .conmouooo huwHoCOmuom saw» .mucaoaoeoo oeuMEOm ooxwe noouo nmufiaouwomom ocoz vw mH oouoaou auoaxc< undo acoeuosno< aw m ma mannouo Hoowooe Ou ooumHou auwao20muom uconuuaoso 0:02 «a o nowadaouauuo Huaxmm ucmocoaoo mm m «a acosuooue acauooaoo: nxoos ououm mousuoom Ho0wcfiau uwoocmoao oo< xom omou modum acouuso acoEuooue Hon ucoowuwcoam nocuo ucouuoo A.o.ucoov n «Home S6 sufficiently disturbed to warrant the additional diagnosis of Borderline Personality organization (see Kernberg, 1975). The Nondepressed Clients generally presented more diffuse clinical pictures. Few of these clients obviously fit other "textbook" descriptions of other psychiatric disorders. Rather, 11 of these participants were assessed as presenting the sort of mixed symptomatology associated with the diagnoses of adjustment reaction or personality disorder. These were the sort of clients many therapists refer to as "garden variety neurotics." Nine of the Non- depressed Clients reported current or prior difficulty with depressive mood or symptoms. Yet, as can be seen from Table 4, the mean Beck Depression Inventory score of 6.50 of the Nondepressed Clients is significantly lower than the mean score of 28.06 of the Depressed Clients. The treatment histories of the two clinical groups were fairly similar. All but two participants from each group had been in counseling for six months or less at the time of their participation. The length of treatment of the Depressed Clients ranged from 3 to 150 weeks, with a median of 10 weeks and a mean of 21.37 weeks. The length of treat- ment of the Nondepressed Clients ranged from 3 to 68 weeks, with a median of 16 weeks and a mean of 19.68 weeks. The difference between means is not statistically significant (F = .026, p = .86, df = 31). Eleven of the Depressed Clients and 12 of the Nondepressed Clients had received professional help for emotional or psychiatric difficulties Table 4 57 Comparison of Groups on the Beck Depression Inventory Group Mean BDI Range SD Depressed Clients Nondepressed Clients Normals 16 16 16 19-40 6.10 0-13 4.60 t-test Comparisons (separate variance estimate) Comparison E value df .E probability Depressed Clients 15.08 19 .000 vs. Normals Depressed Clients vs. Nondepressed 11.28 28 .000 Clients 58 prior to their current treatment. Three from the former group and seven from. the latter had at some time Ibeen hospitalized for psychiatric reasons. In only one instance, however, (Nondepressed Client #10) had this hospitalization been a part of a client's current treatment. Eleven of the depressed and four of the nondepressed clients were cur- rently taking antidepressants or minor tranquilizers. None of the client participants reported having ever received electro-convulsive therapy. Psyphiatrically Normal Participants Sixteen psychiatrically normal adults were selected from a pool‘of 26 volunteers who had been recruited from the general population of Coles and Edgar Counties, Illinois. The 26 volunteers were recruited from a variety of sources including educational institutions, civic organizations, a newspaper advertisement and referrals from acquaintances of the principal investigator. Table 5 presents the referral sources of the included participants. Fourteen volunteers had never met the principal investigator prior to their participation and two volunteers could be described as "passing acquaintances" of the principal investigator. Ten volunteers were dropped from the study either because they failed to meet the inclusion criteria or because no suitable match could be found for them in the client groups. Table 5 59 Referral Sources of Psychiatrically Normal Participants Source Number Newspaper Advertisement in the 4 Charleston Times Courier Referrals from acquaintances of 4 the principal investigator Eastern Illinois University, 3 Charleston, Illinois Mattoon Area Adult Education Center, 2 Mattoon, Illinois Lakeland College, Mattoon, Illinois 1 Physician's Office, Charleston, Illinois 1 Inclusion Criteria for Psychiatrically Normal Participants The inclusion criteria for the Psychiatrically Normal group (to be referred to as "Normals") are outlined below. A volunteer is to be included in the Psychiatrically Normal group if and only if he/she meets criteria 1 through 5 Is between 20 and 60 years of age. Reports none of the following problems during the past two years: severe prolonged anxiety symptoms, depression of clinical intensity, alcoholism or problem drinking, serious marital or sexual problems, hospitalization for psychiatric reasons. 60 3. Has not been hospitalized for psychiatric reasons since the age of 21. 4. Scores below 10 on the Beck Depression Inventory. 5. Has not been separated from his/her spouse for frequent or extended periods during the past 3 months. Although the principal investigator had specified that she was seeking persons who had 922 experienced emotional difficulties or psychiatric symptoms during the past two years, several volunteers apparently overlooked or dis- counted this. Six volunteers were excluded because they either reported pronounced anxiety or depressive symptoms or scored over 10 on the Beck Depression Inventory. The 16 volunteers included in this normal control group appear to have been psychologically healthy. None reported either any history of psychiatric hospitalization or any outpatient treatment during the past two years. One volunteer (#15) said she had contemplated seeking marital therapy but had decided this was not necessary. Another woman (#2) reported occasional difficulties with tension and anxiety headaches, but not so severe as to warrant profes- sional help. None of the Normal participants reported taking antidepressants or tranquilizers. These volunteers' Beck Depression Inventory scores ranged from 0 to 8, with a mean of 3.5 (see Table 4). 61 Matching Participant Groups The three groups were matched on sex distribution, age, length of marriage, and social class. Because each participant reported on his/her spouses', as well as his/her own, behavior and emotions, the groups were matched in terms of the age and social status of the couple rather than of the individual participant. A couple's age was defined as the average age of the husband and wife. A couple's social class was determined by the husband's occupation and educa- tion, in accordance with the Two Factor Index of Social Position (Hollingshead, 1965). The criteria for matching couples are outlined below: Criteria for Matching Couples Two couples will be defined as matched if and only if: 1. The average ages of both couples fall within ten years of each other (average age = husband's age + wife's age /2). 2. The lengths of the two marriages fall within the same range: a) under 2 years; b) 2-7 years; c) 8- 15 years; d) 16-25 years; and e) over 25 years. 3. The couples differ by no more than one class level on the Hollingshead Two Factor Index of Social Position. The Two Factor Index utilizes occupa- tion and education to determine an individual and his/her family's social standing in the community 62 (Hollingshead, 1965). This index demarcates five social classes. Class I is composed of the families of the business and professional elite, that is, of the bank presidents, physicians, lawyers, judges, and executives of large busi- nesses in the community. Heads of Class I households are college and professional school graduates. Families in Class I are usually afflu- ent and often enjoy the privileges associated with inherited wealth. Class II is composed of house- holds headed by lower ranking professionals and business managers; that is, by personnel managers, office managers, minor government officials, librarians, teachers, pharmacists, and owners of medium-sized businesses. Usually the heads of Class II families have acquired some university level education. These families typically are prosperous and upwardly mobile. Class III families are probably what many politicians have in mind when referring to "average, middle-income families.” The heads of Class III households include administrative personnel, small business- men and minor professionals such as insurance agents, shop managers, credit and service man- agers, owners of bakeries or groceries, roofing or painting contractors, commercial artists and travel agents. They have probably completed high 63 school and may have acquired some post-secondary education as well. Class IV families are often referred to as "blue collar," "working class.” Heads of Class IV households are clerical workers, and skilled and semi-skilled workers such as bank tellers, bill collectors, electricians, welders, machinists, roofers, taxi-drivers and foundry workers. Typically they have received, at most, a high school education. Families in Class V on the Two Factor Index belong to what some social scientists might call the ”working poor" or the ”socially marginal." These households are headed by men and women whose jobs, if they are employed, require little skill or education. They work as janitors, domestics, construction laborers or dishwashers. They are unlikely to have graduated from high school (Hollingshead & Redlich, 1958, pp. 84-135). Because the Depressed Clients group was the one of interest in this study, couples from the Nondepressed Clients and Normals groups were matched to them. Table 6 presents the matched sets of couples and the sex of partici- pant, age, length of marriage, and social class of each couple. The actual matching of couples met the criteria reasonably’ well. The 'within group sex distribution *was equivalent across groups as well. Three participants in the c 0 ~ 3 sub 64 N v NN N NH N N NN N NH N NH NN N NH H NH NN : HH N NN HN z N N NN NN N NH N v NN N N N N NN N H N N NN N NH N N NN 2 NH N H NN 2 HH N H NN 2 NH N H NN N H N H NN N NH q H HN N NH 4 N . NN N NH N N NN N NH N N NN N HH N NH «N N N N NH NN N NH N NH HN N NH H N NN N N N N «N N N N H HN N N N NN NN N N N N NN : N N NN NN N N N N HN N NH N N NN N N N NH NN N N v .N NN N N N N HN N v N N NN N N N N N. N NH N N NN N NH N N N. N N N NN NN N N N NN NV 2 N N NN N. N N N N NN 2 NH N N NN N N N H NN z N N HN N. N N N NH NN N NH 4 NH NN N N N NH NN N N N NH «N N N N N HN 2 H mum oowuuo: mod xom oaosoo mum oowuuoz out xom oaosou mum oofiuuoz out xow oaosou numo» muoo» muoow oaoeuoz oucowau oooaouooocoz avocado oommouooo unsouo amount moamooo mcwcouoz N «Hams marr mean and back Year. III ( 65 Depressed Clients and Normals groups each were men; four participants in the Nondepressed Clients were men. This low percentage of men in the Depressed sample is consistent with the well established finding that women outnumber men by a ratio of at least two to one in seeking treatment (Lehman, 1970; Weissman & Klerman, 1977; Woodruff, Goodwin & Guze, 1974). A statistical comparison of the groups also demon- strated that they were equivalent on the age, length of marriage and social status variables. Table 7 presents the mean, standard deviation and range, as well as the F ratios and their probabilities for the comparison of groups on each background variable. The couples were, on the average, 34 years old, married for 9 to 11 years, and in either Class III or IV on the Two Factor Index. Procedure Briefing Clinicians and Establishing Diagnostic Agreement Prior to data collection, the principal investigator met with the staff of the cooperating mental health centers and with the two psychologists in private practice in order to establish rapport and to familiarize these people with the research project. The agenda for these meetings was two-fold: to acquaint the clinicians with the design and purpose of the study, and with its procedures and 66 NH.H NuH NN.N NH mHmeNoz NN. NuN NN.N NH mucoHHo cmmmmudmccoz NN. NN\N NNN.H NN. NIN NN.N NH muchHo ommmmummo mmMHU HMHOOm NN.HH NNuH NN.HH NH NHNsNoz NN.N NNuH NN.N NH NuamHHo NNNNNNchcoz NN. NN\N NNN. NN.NH NNIH HN.HH NH mucmHHo Nommmumoo Amado» :NH omowuuoz mo cwmcog HN.NH NN-N.NN NN.NN NH mHmeNoz NN.N NN-N.NN NN.NN NH munoHHo oommmummncoz NN. NN\N NHH. NN.NH NN-N.NN NN.NN NH NucmHHo NNNNNNNNN om< .ooum m mm oNuom M coNumN>oo cocoa coo: m moouw NNNNNNHN ¢>oza Nmsmco mmmao Howoom oco Homowuumz mo camcoquom« co undouu\moamdoo mo :ONNHMQEOU N mHnme 67 questionnaire materials and to instruct them in the applica- tion of the diagnostic inclusion criteria for the two client groups. Scheduling constraints limited these briefing sessions to one to one and a half hours. The principal investigator explained that the aim of the study was to examine the marital relationships of out- patients who fit a diagnosis of primary unipolar depression and to compare these relationships to those of outpatients who received some other diagnosis. Consequently, the investigator was seeking only currently married clients as volunteers. Copies of a handout entitled "Target Groups" (see Appendix A), which specified the inclusion criteria for the two client groups were given to each referring clinician. The principal investigator asked clinicians to periodically review their caseloads and invite any of their clients who fit the inclusion criteria to participate in this study. While the investigator asked that the thera- pists refer as many clients as possible, as early in their treatment as possible, she assured these therapists that she, of course, left the decision on the suitability and timing’ of any' client's participation to the therapist's discretion. This interview and questionnaire study, it was explained, had been designed to be as nonthreatening and even potentially useful and informative as possible to both the participants and their therapists. Participation would require about two hours of a client's time, for which s/he 68 would receive a $5.00 "Thank you" payment as well as feed- back information. Another handout entitled "Guidelines for Briefing Client Participants" (see Appendix B) detailed the salient information about the principal investigator and the study to be mentioned in recruiting any client's participa- tion. The cooperating clinicians were urged to refer to this handout when recruiting clients. In order to minimize her demands on frequently over- worked clinicians, the principal investigator asked only that they brief their clients on the nature and requirements of the study and then secure an interested client's permis- sion to give his/her name and telephone number to the principal investigator. She would assume full responsibil- ity for scheduling appointments with the client. Moreover, the principal investigator would take the initiative and periodically telephone therapists to inquire about client referrals. Cooperating therapists were assured that the principal investigator would in no way intervene in a client's treat- ment program. She would be pleased, however, to provide diagnostic impressions and questionnaire feedback which a therapist might find helpful and/or interesting. An inter- pretive summary of each client's questionnaire responses could be provided to either the client and/or his therapist, at the therapist's discretion. Upon meeting with a client the principal investigator would ascertain his/her verbal, though not written, consent to discuss interview and 69 questionnaire data with the referring clinician. The principal investigator also jprovided. the clinicians ‘with copies of the questionnaires their clients would be asked to complete and commented briefly on the purpose and merits of each. The second task of these briefing sessions was to establish inter-rater agreement in diagnosing primary uni- polar depression. This proved to be difficult, as there was seldom enough time to reconcile all differences between the principal investigator's diagnostic criteria and a staff member's often informal, and occasionally idiosyncratic, definitions of depression, anxiety neuroses, personality disorders, etc. The principal investigator was able, how- ever, to discuss in detail the research criteria for a differential diagnosis of primary depression and to point out that this was a more stringent definition than might commonly be used by clinicians. In addition, cooperating clinicians were given c0pies of "Diagnosing Primary Unipolar Depression" (see Appendix C), which details the criteria for diagnosis according to guidelines provided by Woodruff, Goodwin and Guze (1974). Where time permitted, staff members were also asked to apply the criteria to several case histories drawn from textbooks and the research literature on depression (see Appendix D) and to discuss their diagnostic decisions with the principal investigator. These information discussions were intended to clarify mis- understandings, as well as to point out difficulties in 70 applying the criteria, rather than to arrive at a precise measure of inter-rater reliability. Contacting, Interviewing and Testing Client Participants The principal investigator periodically telephoned cooperating therapists in search. of referrals. In. most instances the clinician provided diagnostic information in making the referral. Upon securing an interested client's name and telephone number, the principal investigator tele- phoned him/her to introduce herself and to schedule interview and questionnaire sessions. In order to insure the client's informed consent, the investigator first explained in detail what would be asked of each participant and what would be offered him/her in return. She explained that the aim of the interview she would conduct was strictly to understand the nature of a participant's problems rather than to intervene in the client's treatment. Participants were also assured that all information they provided would be held in strictest confidence. The interview and ques- tionnaire session(s) would be scheduled at the participant's (and in half the cases, the spouse's) convenience. Seven- teen clients were interviewed at the referring agency, 13 at the client's home, one at the client's business office, and one at the principal investigator's home. Interview appointments began with the principal investigator's reading the following orientation protocol. 71 Orientation Protocol Hello, I'm Paula McNitt, and as you know I am conduct- ing a research study on marital relationships. Before we begin, we need to review what the study involves, and what your rights are as a participant. 1. If Participant is a Client or Spouse First I'll be asking you about yourself and the problems you have been experiencing lately. My purpose here is solely to get clear on why you have requested help here at the Mental Health Center. I'm not here to do therapy--that's your counselor's job. This interview should last from one-half to one hour, at most. For all Participants You'll (also) be asked to complete several question— naires. One questionnaire asks for some simple back- ground information about yourself, your job, your schooling, etc. Another questionnaire asks about how you are feeling right now. The others ask about your relationship with your husband/wife during the last three months. These questions are straightforward; there's nothing sneaky about them. There are no right or wrong answers, so please answer them as honestly as you can. In all there are 300 questions of the ”multiple choice” type. These take about one hour to complete. 72 Please understand that all your answers will be kept strictly confidential. No one will be told about these unless you give me permission, in writing, to inform a particular person. If for any reason you wish to with- draw from this project, you may do so--that is your right. When you (and your spouse) have completed the ques- tionnaires, you will be paid $5.00 for your participa- tion. This is my small way of saying "Thank you." In addition, if you request, I will be glad to schedule a "feedback session" with you in which we can summarize and review your answers. This can be held in about four weeks. Eventually I can provide a summary of the findings of the entire study. Do you have any questions? Before we proceed, I need your signature on this consent form. Following this, each participant was asked to sign the "Departmental Research Consent Form" of Michigan State University's Department of Psychology (see Appendix E). The several participants referred from the Mental Health Clinic of' the ‘Veterans .Administration. Medical. Center, Danville, Illinois, were asked to sign an additional consent form which met that agency's specifications (see Appendix F). 73 The assessment interview which followed was a semi- structured one in which the investigator queried the client about his/her current and recent emotional state, probed for symptoms of depression and. anxiety disorders, and. asked about. his/her treatment. history' and. reasons for seeking professional help. The "Guidelines for Interviewing Clients" presented in Appendix G indicate the directions in which interviewer guided the client. After the client finished his/her story, so to speak, the principal investi- gator initiated a more structured series of questions by which she checked for current or past symptoms and problems not already mentioned by the client and ascertained the details of his/her treatment history. This line of ques- tioning, which she called the "Laundry List of Problems," followed the checklist of the Summary of Diagnostic Data, samples from which are presented below. The complete format is in Appendix H. (Are any of these a problem for you now or have they been in the past?) I. Depressive Symptoms-~Current A. Dysphoric Mood depressed, sad, blue, hopeless, discouraged, irritable, fearful, worried, cried a lot II. C. Drug Dependency (include all street or prescrip- tion drugs) 74 current Yes No past Yes No IV. C. Kinds of Treatment Received individual therapy family group therapy medical/somatic marital In many instances the principal investigator subse- quently consulted with a client's therapist to clarify vague or missing information and to exchange additional diagnostic impressions. During the interview the principal investigator took extensive notes and completed the Summary of Diagnostic Data. When a client's spouse participated as well, a con- joint interviewr was conducted. In. these interviews the partners typically alternated in reporting on his/her own problems and the partner's problems, as well as on other shared difficulties. The interviewer also queried each partner individually on his/her symptoms and treatment history, as specified by the Summary of Diagnostic Data. At the conclusion. of the interview, if scheduling permitted, the participant was given a packet of question- naires to complete. In 23 of 32 cases both the interview and questionnaires were completed in the course of one meeting. An additional appointment had to be scheduled for nine clients, usually within a week of the interview session. The questionnaire packet contained an instruction 75 sheet and copies of the General Information Sheet (prepared by the author, (see Appendix I)), the Beck Depression Inventory (see Appendix J), the Marital Communication Inventory (see Appendix K), the Love Scale (see Appendix L) and Series A and B of the Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (see Appendix M). The principal investigator read through the following instruction sheet with each client participant. Instructions for Participants (A) This packet contains several questionnaires. One questionnaire asks for general information about your age, education, occupation, marital history. Three question- naires ask about your relationship with your husband/wife during the past three months. There are a lot of questions, so you certainly should spend no longer than a moment on each one! The best answer is probably the one that comes to mind first. It is most important that you answer the ques- tions as honestly as you can. Please try to answer all of the questions, but if you can provide no reasonable or sensible answer for one, omit it. I am interested in what m have to say about your marriage, not in what your husband or wife, mother-in-law, daughter, or your best friend thinks you should say. A few of the questions are about things most of us consider to be private. Please don't worry about this. Be assured that your answers will be kept in strictest confidence. Neither I nor your 76 counselor will discuss your answers with anyone unless you request, in writing, that we do so. Thank you very much for your participation. Paul C. McNitt, M.A. She reviewed the format. of each. questionnaire and explained how to use the separate answer form. for the Structural Analysis of Social Behavior. Clients were advised that they might complete the questionnaires in any order they preferred. The principal investigator remained with the participants while they worked on the question- naires in order to answer questions and to discourage con- sultations between husbands and wives who were completing these questionnaires at the same time. Whenever a client asked about the meaning or intent of an item, the author suggested that s/he answer in accordance to his/his own interpretation. After the participant had completed the packet, the principal investigator asked whether s/he had any concerns or questions about the questionnaire items. She then thanked the client for his/her participation and gave him/ her a handwritten thank-you note which contained the $5.00 payment. She also advised clients that individualized feed- back would be available to him/her and/or his/her therapist in about a month, and offered to mail the client a summary of the findings when they became available. 77 Recruiting and Testing Psychiatrically Normal Participants The volunteers forming the psychiatrically normal group were recruited from several sources. Several were recruited from one of three educational institutions, from the clientele of a large medical family practice in Charleston, Illinois, and from civic organizations in Coles County, Illinois. In these cases, recruiting involved either publically posting or reading aloud an announcement which stated that the principal investigator was seeking married persons aged 20 to 60 who had not experienced psychiatric or pronounced emotional difficulties during the past two years who would be willing to complete question- naires about their marriage in return for a $5.00 "Thank you payment." Appendix L provides a sample recruitment protocol. Interested. persons ‘were asked to leave their names and telephone numbers on a sign-up sheet: the prin- cipal investigator would contact them. Volunteers who had been referred to the principal investigator by her acquaintances had been given this same information somewhat more informally. During the course of data collection it became appar- ent that the "normal" volunteers were noticeably older and better educated and more affluent than the client partici- pants. In order to attract younger persons who might find a $5.00 payment an incentive to volunteer the principal placed 78 the following advertisement in the Charleston (IL) _T_i_r_n_<-2_s_ Courier, twice a week for two weeks: Local psychologist seeks young married persons to complete several questionnaires. Will pay $5.00. For more information please call 345- 5039. This advertisement proved quite effective in attracting precisely the sort of young, lower income couples the principal investigator was seeking. During the first telephone contact with a potential participant the principal investigator introduced herself and explained that her study was investigating the marriages of people who had been in "good mental health" during the past two years. In order to allay any volunteer's doubt or fears about the integrity of the principal investigator and her project, she explained that the study was being con- ducted in fulfillment of one of the requirements for the Ph.D. degree in Psychology from Michigan State University. Two faculty committees at that institution had approved the study as reasonable and "safe for human beings" (investiga- tor's words). The principal investigator reviewed what would be asked of participants and what would be offered them in return. Appointments were scheduled at the volun- teer's convenience. These questionnaire sessions were held at either the volunteer's home or a meeting room at Eastern Illinois University, Lakeland College or the Mattoon Area 79 Adult Education Center. Five spouses of the 16 Normals volunteers included in the study participated as well. Questionnaire sessions began with the principal investigator's reading the relevant section of the "Orienta- tion Protocol” (see previous section) and securing the participant's signature on the "Department Research Consent Form." She next gave each participant a questionnaire packet and asked that s/he read the following instruction sheet along with her. Instructions for Participants (B) This packet contains six questionnaires. One asks for some general information about you; a second asks about specific problems you may have experienced during the past two years; the third asks about how you are feeling right now. The remaining three questionnaires ask you your rela- tionship with your husband/wife during the past three months. There are a lot of questions, so you certainly should spend no longer than a moment on each one! The best answer is probably the one that comes to mind first. It is most important that you answer the questions as honestly as you can. Please try to answer all of the questions, but if you can provide no reasonable or sensible answer for one, omit it. I am interested in what ygg have to say about your marriage, not what your husband or wife, mother-in-law, daughter, or best friend thinks you should say. A few of the questions ask about things most of us consider to be 80 private. Please don't worry about this! Be assured that all of your answers will be kept in strictest confidence. Then, too, I am most interested in how the participants a§_a gggnp answer these questions, so I will not be paying atten- tion to how any one person answers any one question. Thank you very much for your participation. Paula C. McNitt, M.A. The questionnaire packet given these participants differed from that given the client participants only in that the former included an additional sheet entitled ”Survey of Problems" (see .Appendix 0). This checklist, prepared by the author, queried participants about current or recent symptoms, emotional and relationship problems. The principal investigator proctored these question- naire sessions for the same reasons and in the same manner as she did sessions with the client participants. Upon completing the questionnaires each participant was asked about any concerns or questions s/he had about the study, thanked, given a handwritten "thank you" note containing the $5.00 payment and offered both individualized feedback as well as a summary of findings to be prepared later. Feedback In most instances the interpretive summary of a client participant's questionnaire responses was forwarded to the client's therapist, rather than to the client him/herself. 81 Such an arrangement. was usually the therapist's choice. Three client participants attended feedback sessions with the principal investigator and one received a written feed- back summary as she requested. Those few of the normal participants who requested individualized feedback, received it in meetings with the principal investigator. A written summary of the findings will be sent shortly to all cooper- ating agencies and to those participants who requested such information. The following flow-chart summarizes the procedures the principal investigator followed in the conduct of this study: Client Participants Normal Participants I. Recruiting Volunteers A. Contact referring A. Secure names and therapists for names telephone numbers of and telephone numbers interested persons of interested clients. from: - In some cases, 1. Sign-up sheets referring clinician a. posted at provides diagnostic local schools information. & colleges b. circulated at meetings fol- lowing reading of recruitment protocol II. 82 Telephone volunteers to: 1. Introduce self and study. 2. Acquaintances of the principal investigator 3. Responses to newspaper adver- tisement 2. Brief volunteers on requirements, benefits, rights etc. of participation. 3. Schedule appointment. Data Collection A. Read Orientation protocol and secure the volun- teer's informed, written consent. Interview client B. participant (and Spouse, if avail- able) Proctor question- C. naire session Probe for client's questions/concerns, if any. Arrange for feedback. Proctor question- naire session Probe for partici- pant's questions/ concerns if any. Arrange for feedback 83 E. In some cases, contact referring clinician re: diagnostic questions III. Provide Feedback A. Individualized Feedback 1. Summary mailed--or 1. Provided orally presented orally to some partici- to clinician pants 2. Provided orally to some client participants B. Overall Findings of Study--summary to be mailed to participants and referring clinicians. Instruments All participants in this study completed the General Information Sheet, prepared by the author, the Beck Depres- sion Inventory (Beck, 1972), Series A and B of the Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (Benjamin, 1979), a modified version of the Love Scale (Swensen & Gilner, 1968), and the Marital Communication Inventory (Bienvenu, 1978). The husbands and wives in the Normals group also completed the ”Survey of Problems" prepared by this author. This brief self-report form asks about emotional and relationship problems and psychiatric symptoms experienced during the past two years. 84 The Beck Depression Inventogy (BDI) is a self-report instrument which measures depth of depression. Its con- struction presupposes that depth of depression varies directly with the number and intensity of depressive symp- toms. The BDI includes 21 clinically derived categories of attitudes and physical symptoms specific to depression (Beck, 1972, p. 189). Each category item consists of four or five graded self-report statements. Numerical values for these range from 0, for neutral, to 3, for maximal severity. Individual scores, which range from 0 to 63, are determined by the number and severity of symptoms reported. Beck (1972, p. 196) reports mean scores associated with clinical ratings of depression for a psychiatric population. These range from 18.7 for mildly depressed patients to 30 for severely depressed. The body of data on the reliability and validity of the BDI is impressive (Beck, 1972, pp. 184-202). The scale is internally consistent; split half reliability is .93, and item total correlations range from .31 to .68. Concurrent validity of the inventory has been established by its high correlations with other measures of depression. Beck (1972, pp. 197-198) reports correlations of .65 and .75 between BDI scores and clinicians' ratings of severity of depression. BDI scores also correlate well with scores on Lubin's Depression Adjective Check List (r = .66) and the MPI D- Scale (r = .75) (Beck, 1972, p. 198). Unlike these latter two, the BDI discriminates between depression and anxiety. 85 Moreover, changes in BDI scores can predict changes in clinicians' ratings (Beck, 1972, p. 198). Construct validity of the inventory has been supported by confirmation of several hypotheses. These are: depressed patients are more likely to report dreams with themes of deprivation, failure, and frustration, to identify with the "loser" in projective tests dealing with success and failure, to have a history of childhood deprivation, and to respond to experi- mentally induced failure with a disproportionate drop in self-esteem. In short, the Beck Depression Inventory is as good a measure of depression as one can reasonably expect of a brief, easily administered self-report questionnaire, and as such, was utilized in this study. The Structural Analysis of Social Behavior (SASB) (Benjamin, 1974, 1979a) is both a model of and methodology for assessing interpersonal behavior. Benjamin has developed this complex and sophisticated model over the past 17 years (Benjamin, 1980, personal communication) and has presented extensive research data on its validity (Benjamin, 1974), as well as reported on its usefulness as a clinical assessment tool (Benjamin, 1979b). The SASB, which elabor- ates on earlier models of Leary and of Schaefer (Benjamin, 1974), presupposes certain explicit logical and mathematical properties, which shall be discussed. SASB's explicit theoretical model and the wealth of research data supporting it, and the sophisticated computer- ized techniques available for analysis of individual 86 protocols, make it a most attractive tool for clinical research. The SASB questionnaires generate multiple measures of affiliation, hostility, autonomy, dominance, and submission, and thus a more complex, and consequently more accurate picture of a respondent's perceived relationship with a significant other. The several measures of autonomy and of dominance-submission are of particular interest in a study which hypothesizes mutual dependency as a defining feature of the depressed client's marriage. A Description of the Model: The model (summarized by Figure 1) locates any social behavior on one of three diamond-shaped surfaces. The top (first) surface describes behaviors that are performed to or for another person. This plane is referred to as the "Parent-like" and "Focus-on- Other” surface. The middle (second) surface describes behaviors that are done to or for the self (in response to another's Surface 1 behavior). This plane is referred to as the ”Child-like" and "Focus on Self" surface. These first two planes implicitly define an active-passive dimension. The bottom (third) surface is the introject plane. It describes intrapersonal attitudes and behaviors. The behaviors on this surface were deduced to be the conse- quences of introjecting the parent-like behaviors of Surface 1. Because this study focuses on interpersonal behavior it will assess only Surface 1 and 2 behaviors. Each surface is defined by the horizontal, affilia- tion, axis and the vertical, autonomy-interdependence, axis. 87 120 Enancipate Go away now 128 118 You can do it Exclude 127 117 Encourage divergence Isolate 126 . "r---1'1‘ 116 Listen. equalitarian 115 Explore, let discover Neglect. ignore 125 Bluff. illogical 124 Abandon. reject 123 114 Confirm, praise 113 Play, allow peer play Deprive 122 112 Smile, greet war-1y 111 Kiss. heal. groan Starve. poison 121 110 Embrace. tender.touch Murderous attack 130 141 Support. cradle. nurse Injure 131 Frighten 132 142 Indulge. T.L.C. Iestrain. overprotect 133 143 Protect. keep conpany Shout. criticize 134 144 Reasoned persuasion Threaten 135 145 Stimulate. teach Exploit 136 146 Overindulge Authoritarian 137 L. .__J 147 Intrude Shale, guilt control 138 148 Possessive Doeinate 140 220 Be eoancipated Flee, withdraw 228 218 Unassaultive assert Coupete. try one-up 227...H ._ . 217 Individualistic Io input. no response 226 Stinulate self 225 C:.. 216 Cooperative 215 Reveal, discover Defy. suspect 224 Distrust. grieve 223 - 214 Display. court Pl 213 Play, peer play Touper tantrul 222 212 Approach, snile. understand 211 Accept. grooe. heal Disgust. refuse. spit 221 Suffer. disaffiliate 230 210 Hug. affiliate no not touch no 231 I : 241 Accept. nurse. sleep ——i-—I—p-1 Rigid. hide, grinace 232 242 Flower child Cling, annoy 233 I l 243 Trust Cringe. defend. whine 234 Present. ield. appease 235 entfu coeply 236 244 Comply willingly 245 Absorb. imitate 246 Incompetent om routines 237 1...... 247 Overconforla, defer flaky conply 238 248 Satellite I. lounted. sub-it 240 320 Elancipate self new In 328 318 Self-confident. reliant Need Exclude self, one-down 327 317 Self actualize Isolate self 326 . N.....r.? 316 Fair, Just Neglect. ignore self 325 Put self on 324 Reckless 323 315 Explore self 314 Confirm self 313 Entertain self Self deprivation 322 312 Helcone self 311 Real. groom self Starve. poison self 321 310 Self love Self hate. suicide 330 anure self 331 341 Self support —d——_ Frighten self 332 342 Self care Apathy 333 343 Protect self Criticize self 334 No products. nihilistic 335 Self sacrifice 336 344 Dignified, respect self 345 Self taught. accomplished 346 Overindulge self, addict Person - cause 337 347 Self preoccupied Accuse self, guilt 338 348 Self possessed I an my own master 340 Figure 1. Map of SASB Model. 88 The poles of these axes are love (right) and hate (left) and emancipate/be emancipated (top) and dominate/submit (bottom), respectively. The axes define four' quadrants. These are numbers I to IV in accordance with the Cartesian tradition of assigning I to the upper right quadrant and proceeding counterclockwise. They are labelled in the following way (Benjamin, 1977): Quadrant Focus on Other Focus on Self I Encourage Friendly Enjoy Friendly Autonomy Autonomy II Invoke Hostile Take Hostile Autonomy Autonomy III Hostile Power Hostile Comply IV Friendly Influence Friendly Accept The behaviors in the bottom two quadrants are saturated with control in the sense of either dominance or submission. Each surface has also been divided into octants which are similar to those in Leary's model (Benjamin, 1979). Figure 2 presents Surfaces 1 and 2 divided into octants. The names presented here for each octant have been supplied by this author. Each of the 36 behavior points on a surface consists of complementary proportions of affiliation and inter- dependence. The poles (110, 120, 130, 140) represent primitive behaviors: tender sexuality, emancipate, annihilating attack, dominate. Points further away from the poles represent less primitive behaviors. If one starts at ..-- ~-----H. --' Uncarinfly Ietgo128 89 120 Endorse freedom 11 I 8 Encourage separate identity 11 'm. “fiend not there 125 I ”5 Carefully. "MY cmsrder —— 8 Neflect interests. needs 125 I "5 F'Wfld‘v ““0" a. "logical initiation 124 114 Show ernoath'oc understanding _...H...H 113 Confirm as OKasis Lt Sum cut our 322 112 Stroke. southe. cairn dismiss, rem 121,1" Wiffl'ilv Mm Annihilating m 130 —IL 110 Tender $080815” W menaangly 131 141 Frtendly invite W132 51W Punish. take revenge 133 14 rorect. back 09 m. divert. misuse 134 1“ 50ml"- min" b Accuse. blame 135 145 Constructive stimulate Li - 145 Pamper. overindui lntrude. block. restrict 137 — h. “‘7 Benevolent monitor. remind Entoru conformity 138 L— ”—1 “3 5M" W1" b.“ Manage. control 140 S 220 Freely come aid go Cowman" 223—1J1 2180mUentirv. standards ' —' —‘ 217 As n ' (o war-on. nondisclose 225 1 " u! cards on the u Busy with own thing 225 1 215 Openly disclose. reveal ‘ o Noncmtingent reaction 224 214 Clearly express 2 . . Refuse assistance. care 222 212 Relax. now. enjoy Flee. escape. withdraw 221 211 Joyful approach Desperate protest 230 . 210 Ecstatic response | S Wary. fearfu1231 L 241 Follow. maintain contact. ‘ i * k5 Appears. ram 233 243 Ask. trust. count on Urtcornprenending agree 234 i * 244 Accept reason Whine. defend. justify 235 1 L 245 Take in. learn from (H 5011:. act out on 236 356 Cling, deggnd 1 9. ti: comiianoe 237 L. { 247 Defer. overconiorrn Follow ruies. proper 233 k 243 Submerge into m l 3 Yield, submit, give in 240 Figure 2. Map of SASB Octants. 90 any point and moves stepwise within its quadrant, the sub- jective quality of these behaviors changes in the direction of the pole being approached. A behavior point's absolute value on each dimension ranges from 0 to 9, and the sum of these absolute values is always equal to 9. The valences (signs) on both dimensions vary according to the point's quadrant. For example, "pamper, overindulge" (146), located in quadrant IV, has +3 units of affiliation and -6 units of autonomy; "openly disclose" (215), located in quadrant I has +4 units of affiliation and +5 units of autonomy. Every behavior point on Figure I is specified by a three-digit code number. The first (1003) digit locates the surface: 1 = Focus on Other, 2 = Focus on Self, and 3 Introject. The second digit (10s) locates the quadrant, 1 through 4. The third digit (ls) locates the track within the quadrant. These tracks are: (Benjamin, 1979b) Track 0--Primitive basics (poles) Track l--Approach-avoidance Track 2-—Need-fulfillment Track 3--Attachment Track 4--Logic, communication Track 5--Attention to self-development Track 6--Balance in relationship Track 7--Intimacy-distance Track 8--Identify 91 All points on the same track consist of the same proportions of the two dimensions. Opposite behaviors are located at 180 degrees from each other, that is, on the same track in opposite quadrants (I and III, II and IV). These behaviors differ only in their valences on both dimensions. For example, "you can do it fine” (117), with +2 units of affiliation and +7 units of autonomy, is opposite to "intrude, block, resists" (137), with -2 units of affiliations and -7 units of autonomy. Complementary behaviors are those which tend to elicit each other. These are located at the same point (quadrant and track) on the first two surfaces. For example, ”blaming" (135) tends to elicit "defensiveness" (235); "open self-disclosure" (215) tends to elicit "friendly listening" (115). Behaviors from both the ”parent-like” and the ”child-like” surfaces can be elicitors. No one type of behavior is the more responsible in a behavior exchange (Benjamin, 1974). In complementary behavior exchanges each person's behavior is equally responsible in determining the sequence of events. The antithesis of any behavior is the complement of its opposite. For example, the antithesis of ”hostile wall off, nondisclose" (226) is "pamper, overindulge" (146), which elicits "cling, depend" (246). The principles of complementary and antithetical behavior allow for behavioral predictions and for prescriptions for desired behavior change. 92 Benjamin (1974, 1979a) has developed a battery of questionnaires whose items describe each behavior point on Figure 1 with respect to a variety of relationships. This study will utilize two of these: Series A and Series B. Each series consists of 72 items, one for each point on the "Focus on Other" and ”Focus on Self" surfaces. Series A asks the subject to rate the interpersonal behavior of a significant other vis a vis him/herself (the rater). Series B asks the subject to rate his/her own behavior vis a vis the significant other. The item structure is identical on both series. For example: Series A (20) accuses, blames me, tries to get me to admit I'm wrong. Series B (20) I accuse, blame him/her, try to get him/her to admit he/she is wrong. For the sake of clarity, the questionnaire items were modi- fied so as to specify the person and relationship to be rated. For example: Series A (20) My wife blames me, accuses me, tries to get me to admit I am wrong. Series B (20) I blame, accuse my husband, try to get him to admit he is wrong. The questionnaire items appear in random order. Each item is rated on a scale of 0-100. Anchor points are 0 = never, not at all; 50 = sometimes, moderately; 100 = always, 93 perfectly. Subjects are advised to place their ratings at the 10-point intervals. The directions for completing the questionnaires follow Benjamin's (1979), with a few modifi- cations (see Appendix M). As Benjamin has emphasized (Benjamin, 1979a), SASB is a "good faith” measure. Unlike the MMPI it does not attempt to assess or to correct for the test taker's dishonesty or defensiveness. Thus, subjects must be urged to be as honest as possible in reporting their feelings and perceptions. Moreover, the questionnaires tap only the rater's percep- tions of his/her own and others' social behaviors. These perceptions may or may not agree with the objective reality of his/her relationships. Validity Studies Statistical analyses of extensive data drawn from several different subject groups have confirmed the struc- tures of the SASB model. Benjamin (1974) has argued that if the SASB model mirrors the actual organization of social behavior, then subjects should rate items adjacent on a surface similarly, distant items dissimilarly and opposite items, oppositely. Similarity of ratings is assessed in terms of patterns of autocorrelations. The autocorrelation at Lag 1 pairs scores for items immediately adjacent to each other. The autocorrelation at Lag 2 pairs scores for all items two steps apart, and so on for Lags 3 through 35. Correlations for Lags 17 to 35 mirror those for Lags 1-17. 94 Theoretically the variation in autocorrelations across lags should replicate an inverted normal (Z) curve. Correlations are large and. positive at Lags 1, 2 and 3, decrease, approaching zero at Lag 9, where item points are orthogonal, and become increasingly negative as the curve approaches Lag 17, where opposite items are paired. The autocorrelation curves generated from the questionnaire responses of normal adults, in fact, approximate the inverted Z curve. The average correlation between the obtained curves and inverted normal curve were .92 and .97 for samples of 60 and 50 normal adults, respectively (Benjamin, 1974). This correlation may be construed as an index or coef- ficient of internal consistency insofar as it reflects the degree to which a person gives similar ratings to items hypothesized to be similar, opposite ratings to items hy- pothesized to be Opposite and unrelated ratings to items hypothesized to be orthogonal. Interview data from subjects who completed the entire battery of SASB questionnaires indicated that a high coefficient of internal consistency was associated with behavioral stability over time (Benjamin, 1974). A low coefficient of internal consis- tency, Benjamin (1974, 1979a) suggests, is associated with behavioral instability rather than with unreliability of the questionnaires. To support this interpretation, Benjamin (1974, 1979a) cites both interview data as well as her finding that in every set of comparisons psychiatric patients produced a much lower coefficient of internal 95 consistency than did normal adults. Behavioral instability is a frequent, though not universal, concomitant of psychi- atric disorder. The dimensional structure of the model has been supported by circumplex and factor analyses. The SASB model assumes that each of its items taps both affiliation and interdependence, and thus fits a circumplex model, which also presupposes that a questionnaire's items simultaneously tap each of two underlying dimensions to systematically varied degrees. A circumplex is the model of interrelation- ship between two factors such that the factor loadings for every variable stand in the relationship defined by the equation for an ellipse: cza? + kab? = h2 i l c, k, and h are arbitrary constants and ai and bi are the factor loadings of variable i on the first and second factors, respectively. The correlation coefficients rij = a.a. + b.b. can be ordered in a circumplex pattern. This is 1 J l J a correlation matrix wherein the correlations are highest near the main diagonal, and decrease and increase again as one moves away from the diagonal. The inter item correla- tion matrix generated from the ratings of 221 mothers of their 171 normal and 50 psychiatric clinic children fits well with the circumplex pattern (Benjamin, 1974). A factor analysis of the questionnaire responses of 110 adult sub- jects yielded four factors which accounted for 64 percent of 96 the variance. These four factors: affiliation, disaffilia- tion, emancipation, and domination, were replicated in several additional data samples (Benjamin, 1974). A simple transformation reduced these four factors to two: affilia- tion and emancipation. A plot of each item's scores on the two factors proved a reasonable facsimile of Figure 1 (Benjamin, 1974). The principle of complementarity has been confirmed for most pairs of complementary behavior items. Benjamin (1974) reports that the correlations between mothers' (N = 171) ratings of their own Surface 1 behaviors and their rat- ings of their children's Surface 2 behaviors were stat- istically significant for 28 of the 36 pairings. For 19 of the 36 Surface 1 items, the highest correlations with any of the Surface 2 behavior items was with its complement or an item within two steps of the complement. In general, normal adults report a higher degree of complementarity in their relationship than do psychiatric patients (Benjamin, 1979a). Other systematic differences between the protocols of normal adults and those of psychiatric patients have been noted. Generally, the coefficient of internal consistency is higher for normal respondents (r is .90 or higher). Benjamin (1979a) reports a significant negative correlation between Hamilton Checklist ratings of depression and con- sistency of treatment received from a significant other in a sample of aged women. Normal adults' ratings are usually consistent with the dictates of social desirability. These rate aut< and (Be: ways A m. res; ab0\ deri hiS/ rat: Orie deri for Slimm eith t10n dime: 97 raters characterize their relationships as high on friendly autonomy. Psychiatric patients report much more hostility and dominance in their relationships with significant others (Benjamin, 1974, 1979a). Organization and Analysis of SASB Data Questionnaire data can be organized in a variety of ways. The basic unit of analysis is the individual's map. A map can be generated which indicates all of a subject's responses on a given surface, and highlights all of his above median endorsements. For example, two maps can be derived from a subject's responses on Series B: one of his/her Surface 1 ratings and a second of his/her Surface 2 ratings. These maps provide a picture of the interpersonal orientation of the person rated. Additional scores are derived from these maps. The following have been selected for inclusion in this study. 1. Weighted Affiliation and Autonomy Scores: These summarize a rater's perception of the basic orientation of either him/herself or a significant other in a given rela- tionship. The formula for the weighted score on either dimension is: Weighted Score = z Endorsementi x Weighti The weight, which is either positive or negative, is the number of units of the dimension being scored. Weighted affiliation and weighted autonomy scores are computed for sig ind sco pos ind the per the mo wei COR oct. end add 98 each surface assessed by a questionnaire. Weighted scores may be either large or small, and positive or negative in sign. On the affiliation dimension, large positive scores indicate emotional warmth and intimacy; large negative scores indicate discord. On the autonomy dimension, large positive scores indicate autonomy and large negative scores indicate dominance or submission in the relationship. Yet these weighted scores are straightforward measurements of perceived affiliation and autonomy in a relationship only if the ratings from which they were derived were consistent among themselves. That is, as Benjamin. warns (1979a), weighted scores of a subject whose coefficient of internal consistency is below .90 must be interpreted with caution. 2. Octant Scores: Each surface can be divided into octants (see Figure 2). The octant score is the average endorsement for items *within. the octant. The following additional measures have been derived from these clusters: Sum of Octants 1, 2, 9 and 10--Friend1y Autonomy Sum of Octants 3, 4, 11 and 12--Affiliation Sum of 5 and 6--Dominance Sum of 13 and 14--Submission Sum of 7 and 15--Hostility Sum of 8 and 15--Hostile Autonomy The Love Scale The Love Scale assesses the manifestation of the behaviors and feelings of love in a variety of close 99 interpersonal relationships. These include marital, par- ental, friendship and sibling relationships. The scale, now in its fourth version (Swensen & Gilner, 1968), was derived from a pool of 383 statements about love relationships made by a group of 300 college students (Swensen, 1961). Since 1961 the scale has been reduced to 120 items and admin- istered to several large samples (Swensen, 1978). A factor structure which includes verbal expression of affection, self-disclosure, toleration of less pleasant aspects of the loved person, nonmaterial evidence of love, feelings not expressed. verbally, material evidence «of love, has been replicated in several studies whose subjects ranged in age from 18 to 50 years and older. These factors apparently tap attitudes and behaviors not assessed by other measures of personality and interpersonal behavior. Swanson (1978) reports that the correlations between Love Scale scores and scores on the Interpersonal Checklist, the Guilford- Zimmerman Temperament Survey, the Maudsley Personality Inventory, FIRO-B, and the MMPI scored by Leary's system for the dimensions of love and dominance, are, with few excep- tions, below .20. The current form of the Love Scale (Swensen & Gilner, 1968) consists of 120 items divided into six subscales. These are: Subscale Items Verbal expression of affection 1-20 100 Self disclosure 21-40 Toleration of less pleasant aspects of the loved one 41-61 Nonmaterial evidence of love 62-85 Feelings not expressed verbally 86-104 Material evidence 105-120 Scale items are statements about either the rater's feelings and behaviors toward a loved person or the loved person's feelings and behaviors toward the rater. Items are marked "never," "sometimes," and "always," and scored 1 to 3 (or in some cases 3 to 1) accordingly. Raters are asked to assess their relationship at the present time, with "present time" referring to the past few weeks or months. Subscale scores are the sums of the item scores. The overall Love Scale Index is obtained by adding the scores for subscales 1, 2, 3, 4 and 6 and subtracting the score for subscale 5. The score on subscale 5, "unexpressed feelings," is subtracted because the scale has repeatedly been shown to correlate negatively with the remaining five subscales (Swensen, 1978). Scores on the overall Index range from 44 to 284. Swensen (1978) reports the means and standard deviations on each of the subscales for various age groups and various relationships assessed. The test-retest reliability for each subscale has been assessed for subjects of various age groups. Reliabilities for young (18-26) and "middle age" (27-50) subjects range from .77 to .96. 101 Love Scale Scores and the Marital Relationship The Love Scale has been employed in numerous studies of the marital relationship. Swensen (1978) cites at least a dozen of these (many are unpublished studies conducted by his graduate students at Purdue University). Overall, these studies report no reliable differences between husbands and wives in their assessment of their marriages. The amount of love expressed is highest in the early years of a marriage and declines slowly thereafter (Swensen, 1978). Marital satisfaction, as measured by the Burgess-Wallin Marital Satisfaction Scale, correlates significantly and positively with all subscales but "unexpressed feelings." Scores on this subscale correlate significantly negatively with marital satisfaction scores (Swensen, 1978). Couples who requested professional help for their marital problems scored significantly lower on all but the "unexpressed feelings” subscales than did a comparison group of couples reporting no significant marital discord (Fiore & Swensen, 1977). A Modified Version of the Love Scale This study employed a shortened and modified version of the Love Scale. Subscale 5, "unexpressed feelings," has been dropped because the wording of its items is awkward, and because a low score may indicate either the absence of . , .. smiths r......a.fl...-.m«. East... I f 102 affectionate feelings or reticence about such feelings. Item #60: "You tell the loved one you want to marry him" has been eliminated because it makes no sense whatsoever for persons who are already married. Several items have been expanded to make them more relevant and comprehensible for a sample of married subjects. Items have also been reworded so as to specify the "loved one" as either "your husband" or "your wife.” Two forms, one for wives and one for husbands, were used. A total Love Scale score was computed for each participant. The Love Scale was included as an additional measure of affection/affiliation not only because of the impressive array of validity studies, but also because of the spec- ificity and concreteness of its items. This questionnaire asks the respondent to assess his/her marriage in terms of a variety of attitudes, emotions, and overt behaviors, and, as such generates a more detailed picture of his/her marital relationship. The Marital Communication Inventory The Marital Communication Inventory (Bienvenu, 1970, 1978) was designed to assess the patterns and style of communications between marital partners. The 46 items of this inventory question the extent to which partners express their wants and feelings, listen to and understand each other, and discuss their mutual difficulties. All items relate solely to communications between husband and wife, but to the poi eff ran exp riel mid-c disc rape; aged 103 but are answered by partners individually. Two forms, one to be completed by the husband, and one to be completed by the wife, are available. Each item is scored on a four- point, 0 to 3, scale. Favorable responses, those indicating effective communication are scored highest. Total scores range from 0 to 138. The 46 items of the inventory were derived from an experimental 48 item scale given to 344 subjects (172 mar- ried couples). These were predominately white, Protestant, middle-class couples in their late 208. Of the 48 items, 45 discriminated between the upper and lower quartiles of the sample at the .01 level, and one item discriminated at the .05 level. The mean score for this sampLe of 172 couples was 105.78. This score was cross-validated on a second sample of 60 subjects whose mean score was 105.68. The split half reliability (Spearman-Brown) coefficient for this sample of 60 was .93 (Bienvenu, 1970). Bienvenu (1978) reports similar mean scores for a 1973 sample of 155 couples aged 21-25 and 50 couples aged 26-34. The Marital Communication Inventory has been used in a few studies of marital adjustment and psychOpathology. Bienvenu (1970) reports a study of 23 couples receiving marital counseling which found that they scored signifi- cantly lower on the MCI than did a comparison group of well- adjusted. couples. Murphy’ and..Mendelson (1973) report. a correlation of .846 (p = .05) between scores on the Locke- Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale and MCI scores for a group of l beti repc 5am; (Has repc stoc migl stud deri SCOI Tabl auto qual Subj 104 of 30 young couples. Wasli's (1977) study of communication between 16 hospitalized depressed women and their husbands reports a mean MCI score of 79.56. The range for this sample 'was 41-125 and the standard deviation. was 19.89 (Wasli, 1977, p. 46). This score is well below the norm reported by Bienvenu (1978). This brief, and easily under- stood instrument was utilized so that findings in this study might be compared with those of one of the few available studies of' marital communication and depression (Wasli, 1977). Dependent Measures The dependent measures of this study were 18 scores derived from the SASB questionnaires, the Love Scale total score, and the score on the Marital Communication Inventory. Table 8 presents the Operationalization of the variables of autonomy-interdependence, affiliation, hostility, and quality of communication. Each measure was calculated for subjects individually. Predictions The hypotheses stated in the previous chapter have been translated into specific predictions in terms of the operationalized dependent measures presented in Table 8. Hypothesis 1: The marital relationship of depressed client couples is marked by a higher degree of Table 8 105 Operationalization of Dependent Measures Autonomy-Interdependency SASB A. Overall Interdependency 1. Sum of weighted autonomy scores- 11 Surfaces l and 2 Self (Series B) 2. Sum of weighted autonomy scores— 12 Surfaces 1 and 2 Other (Series A) Friendly Autonomy 3. Sum of Octants scores 1, 2, 9, A1 and 10 Self 4. Sum of Octants scores 1, 2, 9, A2 and 10 Other Hostile Autonomy 5. Sum of Octants scores 8 and 16 HA1 Self 6. Sum of Octants scores 8 and 16 HA2 Other Dominance 7. Sum of Octants scores 5 and 6 D1 Self 8. Sum of Octants scores 5 and 6 D2 Other Submission 9. Sum of Octants scores 13 and 14 81 Self 10. Sum of Octants scores 13 and 14 82 Other (table continues) 106 Table 8 (cont'd.) II. III. Affiliation A. Weighted Affiliation SASB 11. Sum of scores for Surfaces 1 and 2 Self 12. Sum of scores for Surfaces 1 and 2 Other Octant Scores SASB 13. Sum of Octants scores 3 and 4 and 11 and 12 Self 14. Sum of Octants scores 3 and 4 and 11 and 12 Other Affiliation Love Scale 15. Total Score on Love Scale Hostility SASB 16. Sum of Octants 7 and 15 Self 17. Sum of Octants 7 and 15 Other Quality of Communication 18. MCI total score 107 interdependence than that of either psychi- atrically normal adults or other psycho- therapy clients. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be lower in I1 and I2 than either NOndepressed Clients or Normals. Hypothesis 1a: Depressed clients relate to each other as autonomous adults less than do normal hus- bands and. wives, and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be lower in A1 and A2 than either Nondepressed Clients or Normals. Hypothesis 1b: Depressed clients and their spouses manifest more dominance, control, submission and deference than do normal husbands and wives, and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be higher on 02 than either Nondepressed Clients or Normals. 2. Depressed Clients will be lower on D1 than either Nondepressed Clients or Normals. Hypothesis 2: Hypothesis 3: Hypothesis 3a: 108 3. Depressed Clients will be higher on S than either Nondepressed Clients or Normals. 4. Depressed Clients will be lower on S than either Nondepressed Clients or Normals. Depressed clients and their spouses invoke and assume hostile autonomy more often than do normal husbands and wives and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be higher on HA1 and HA2 than either Normals or Non- depressed Clients. The marital relationship of depressed clients is less emotionally rewarding for both partners than is that of psychi- atrically normal adults and other psycho- therapy clients. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be lower on Af1 and Af2 than Normals or Nondepressed Clients. Depressed clients and their spouses express less affection and exchange fewer verbal and nonverbal loving behaviors than do normal Hypothesis 3b: Hypothesis 4: 109 husbands and wives, and other psychotherapy clients and their Spouses. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be lower on Af3, Af and AfS than Normals or Non- 4: depressed Clients. Depressed clients and their spouses express more overt hostility and friction than do normal husbands and wives, or other psycho- therapy clients and their spouses. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be higher on H1 and H2 than Normals or Nondepressed Clients. The communication processes between depressed clients and their spouses are more impaired than those between normal husbands and wives and other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. Predictions: 1. Depressed Clients will be lower on C1 than Normals or Nondepressed Clients. CHAPTER 3 RESULTS In order to determine whether the participant groups differed with respect to autonomy, affiliation, hostility and quality of communication within their marriages, a one- way analysis of variance was computed for each of the 18 dependent measures. The specifically hypothesized differ- ences between the Depressed Clients and each of the two control groups were tested by simple two tailed 2 tests. It was expected, however, that these comparisons would entail some redundancy not only because these involved multiple measures of autonomy, affiliation, hostility and quality of communication, but also because these latter variables might well be correlated among themselves. Consequently, a step- wise discriminant function analysis was conducted to determine which subject of dependent measures best discrim- inated. between. groups. The functions derived from ‘this analysis could then be evaluated in terms of how well they classified all of the cases. The first group of hypotheses tested in this study predicted that the Depressed Clients would report less autonomy and more dominance and submission in their marriages than. would the two control groups. Tables 9 through 13 present the between groups comparisons on these 110 111 ouoaauou oosowuo> uuouomon as ouoawuoo cocoauo> ooaooo a macaqau ooooouooocoz nucowao commouooocoz NNv. mV\H s~N. a o> nucowau ooooouooo ooo. mvxa coo.vt o> nucoaao oomoououo naoeuoz odoEuoz mHo. mva sv¢.~i m> nucowao ooooououo ooo. mv\H «v~.mi m> avocado commouooo noun w on m unnuucoo noNN w No m unnuucoo ocooauooaoo unoa i m mv.vN~.v mv Houuu ob.nN~.n mv uoNNm NNN. NH.N NN.NNN.NH N masons coosuon NNN. NH.NH NN.NNN.NN N masons noosuuo bouo M M me no oousom noun M M as no oousom cocoauo> no nemaaont aozuco NN.NN NHnaNoz NN.NN .NHnaNoz m~.m~ oucowau ounuouooocoz om.om macaquo ooonouooocoz. NN.NH nucoHHo oonnuNNoo NN.HN- nocoHHo nonnouooo coo: macho coo: macho museum 1 >80:0u:< ooucoNoz i «H uuom i h50:0u:¢ voucmuoz 1 HH cocoocumuououcn co nmwouo mo coowuooeoo a manna ON oHetE 112 ouoawunu cocoauo> ououoooo an undefined oocowuo> ooaooo a oucoaao ooououooocoz nucoaao commouooocoz NNN. mv\H HN. i o> mucoNHo ooooououo oNo. mv\H «v.~| o> mocowao oommouooo unoauoz naosuoz NNN. NN\H .HH.N- n> nucoHHo connonooo NNN. NNNH .N.v- m> mucuHHo connonooo oouo m up M umouucoo oouo M no N umouucoo macawuooeoo name i w NN.NHN.N N4 NoNNN NN.NNH.N N4 NoNNN NNN. NN.N NN.NNN.N N Nooono coosuon NNNN. NN.N NN.NNN.NH N noaouu comaoom noun M M we . mo oouoom noun M M we no oousom oocoqun> mo nwomnocd anzoco om.mn~ odoEuoz mN.~m~ mHnENoz m~.omH oucowao commouooocoz oo.-~ oucoNHo oummouooocoz NN.NNN nucoHHo oonnmnaoo NN.NNH nucoHHo omnnoNNmo coo: ozone coo: ozone cocoon i aeocoust aaocofiuh i N< uaom I xaocoust Naocuaum i Nd o Neocoustiaao:0Nuh so emaouu no conquomaoo an wands 113 macaqunu uocoNuo> announce an ouoaNuoo oocoauo> ouaooo a nucowao ooooouooocoz nucowao oomoouooocoz NNN. NN\H ..NN.H n> NuaoHHo nonnououo NNH. NN\H .NN.H n> nunoHHo oonnuuooo odoauoz oaoauoz ooo. NNxH «can.m o> mucowau ooomouooo coo. moxH «NN.N m> oucoNNo commoudoo .noNN w up w unnnocoo ooNN w up w unnuocoo ocooauooeou Nook t M mm.HNv.H my . Nouum NN.NNN mv Nouum mooo. HN.N NN.NHH.NN N odooum cooauom mace. ov.N Hm.Hoo.o N museum coozuom oouo M M as no oou30m noun M M me no oouoom oocoquo> mo oNomaoct Nosoco NN.NN maneuoz Nc.un maneuoz vv.mm nucowau oomnouooocoz oo.mv oucuwau oommouooocoz No.No mucoNHO oonnoudoo om.NN nucowau oommoudoo coo: ozone coo: osouo oosoow i oococweoo t No A meow i oococweoo i o oococwaoo co nooouo No canwuomeou Hm manna 114 ouoluuou oosowuo> ououoooo as ouoswuoo cocoauo> ooaooo a nucoaau venouuooocoz avocado oonoouooocoz mmv. mva coN. o> avocado ooooouooo Hoo. m~\H «amm.m n> nucoaao commouooo napauoz oHoEuoz omo. mva ano.~ n> nucowau ooooouooo coo. NN\N NNHN.v m> mucoNNU ooooouooo nono w No m unnuucoo ooNN w No w unnoucoo ncooauooeoo Home l m No.noo mv NoNNm NH.oe~.N mv uoNNm NH. NN.N NN.NNN.H N noaouu consuon NNN. NN.NH NN.NNN.NH N nosoum opossum noun M M oi no oousom nouo M M we no oousom oocowuo> mo owoaaoc< >o3ono om.mv euoeuoz Nn.Hm nameNoz n~.oo avocado ooonuuooocoz oo.Nm mucowau oowoouooocoz NN.NN nucoHHo connouooo NN.NNH nocoHHo nonnouooo coo: ozone coo: macho ooaoom i cownoaensm i «m NHoN . :oHNNHaoom . Hm nnocu>wnoaanam :0 omsouu mo :omwuooeou NH OHQMB .l.f\ I: :NNTVHTAMHEAWW N. H. twang 115 ouelaueo oocoauo> ououuooo as ouoawuoo oocowuo> ooHOOQ a avocado ouoeeuooocoz nucowau commoumoocoz NNN. NN\H NN.H N> nucoHHo oonnouoon NHN. NN\H .NN.N n> NunoHHo connonooo odoauoz madEuoz HNN. NN\H NN.N n> nucoHHo oonnonooo NNN. NN\H .NN.N n> nucoHHo oomnonooo noNN w up w unnuucoo noun M Nu m unnnucoo Noamauooeou amok i w NN.NNH.H N4 NoNNN HN.NNN Ne NoNNN NNN. NN.N NN.NNN.N N Noaoum coosuom NNNN. HN.NH NN.NNN.N N Nosoua coosuom oouo M M as no coupon noun M M me no oousom oocowun> uo mamaaoct aosoco HN.NN NHneuoz NN.NN NHneNoz Nm.mv oucuaao commuuouocoz om.~v mucoaao oommouomocoz NN.HN nucoHHo ooNNoNooo HN.NN nucoHHo connouaoo coo: moouu coo: macho ooooom I HEO:0uo< euaunom i «4: NHoN . Neocousc oHHuNoN . Ha: aeocousc unauooz co omoouu no cONNuomEou nu wanna 116 variables, which were operationalized as measures derived from ratings on Series A and B of the Structural Analysis of Social Behavior. Hypothesis 1 states that the marital relationships of depressed clients are characterized by a higher degree of interdependence than those of either psychiatrically normal adults or other psychotherapy clients. Table 9 compares the groups on measures Il-Weighted Autonomy-Self and Iz-Weighted Autonomy-Spouse. These are measures of perceived overall interdependence within the marriage; that is, these measures take into account both autonomous and liberating and controlling and submissive behaviors. The predictions that the Depressed Clients would rate both themselves (11) and their spouses (12) lower on autonomy than would the other participants were for the most part confirmed. As can be seen from Table 9, the Depressed Clients' mean of -31.56 on I1,_a negative assessment of their own autonomy in the marriage, was significantly lower than the means of the Normals (74.44) and. of the Nondepressed. Clients (50.50) (both ps < .001). The Depressed Clients rated their spouses (I2) as less autonomous in the marriage than did the Normals (t = -2.44, p < .05), but no differently than did the Non- depressed Clients (E.= -.72, p > .45). Hypothesis 1a. states that depressed clients and their spouses relate to each. other as self-determining, self- asserting adults to a lesser degree than do either psy- chiatrically normal adults or other psychotherapy clients 117 and their husbands and wives. The predictions testing this hypothesis were that the Depressed Clients would score lower on dependent measures Al-Friendly Autonomy-Self, and A2- Friendly Autonomy-Spouse, than either comparison group. As can be seen from Table 10, these predictions were for the most part confirmed. The Depressed Clients rated themselves lower on asserting and promoting their own and their spouse's friendly autonomy (A1) than did both groups of control subjects (t = -4.3, p < .001; E = -2.4, p < .05 for Normals and. Depressed. Clients, respectively). In. rating their spouses, the Depressed Clients reported them to be lower on friendly autonomy (A2) than did the Normals (_i._:_ = -2.11, p < .05) but no differently on this measure than did the Nondepressed Clients (3 =-521, p > .80). Hypothesis 1b. states that depressed patients and their spouses manifest greater dominance and submission in their marriages than do either psychiatrically normal adults or other psychotherapy clients and their husbands and wives. The specific predictions testing this hypothesis were that Depressed Clients would report less dominance and greater submission (D1 and $1) on their own part and greater domin- ance and less submission (D2 and $2) on their spouses' part than would either of the other participant groups. As can be seen from Table 11, the between groups differences on the Dominance measures were not those predicted. The Depressed Clients reported higher, rather than lower, dominance levels on their own part than did the Normals (E = 3.85, p < .001) 118 and levels on this measure equivalent to those reported by the Nondepressed Clients (£_= 1.67, p > .10). As predicted, the Depressed. Clients rated their spouses as higher on Dominance (Dz) than did the Normals (E = 5.83, p < .001). Contrary to prediction, however, the difference on this measure between the two clients groups was not statistically significant (3 = 1.78, p > .05). Table 12 presents the com- and S . 1 2 As expected, the Depressed Clients reported higher levels of parison of groups on measures of submissiveness, S submissiveness on their own part (81) than did either the Normals or the Nondepressed Clients (E = 4.11, p < .001; p = 3.93, p < .01, respectively). Contrary to prediction, the Depressed Clients rated their spouses as more, rather than less, submissive than did the Normals (E = 2.02, p < .05). The two client groups did not, however, differ on the $2 measure (3 = .70, p > .45). Hypothesis 2 states that depressed patients and their spouses invoke and assume hostile autonomy more often than do normal husbands and wives or other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. Specifically, this study predicted that the Depressed Clients would score higher on the HA1 and HA2, the SASB measures of hostile autonomy, than would either control group. Three of the four predictions entailed were confirmed. The Depressed Clients rated themselves higher on Hostile Autonomy (HA1) than did either the Normals or the Nondepressed Clients (3 = 4.6, p < .001; E}= 2.57, p < .05, respectively). In assessing their spouses the Depressed 119 Clients reported higher levels of Hostile Autonomy (HA2) than did the Normal participants (5 = 3.39, p < .01), but not higher levels than did the Nondepressed Clients (_t_ = 1.83, p > .05). The second group of hypotheses tested in this study pertain to the expression of affection and hostility. These variables were operationalized in terms of six measures derived from SASB ratings and of the score on the Love Scale. Hypothesis 3 states that the marital relationships of depressed patients are less emotionally rewarding for both. partners than are those of psychiatrically normal adults or of other psychotherapy clients. Tables 14 through 17 present the between groups comparisons of these seven measures. This hypothesis was operationalized in terms of Af -Weighted Affiliation-Self and Af -Weighted Affiliation- 1 2 Spouse. These are measures of overall affiliation which take into account both affectionate, affiliative and hostile, rejecting behaviors. The study predicted that the Depressed Clients would score lower on both Af1 and Af2 than would either control group. As Table 14 indicates, the pre- dicted between group differences were statistically reliable only for the Depressed Clients vs. Normals comparisons. The former group rated themselves (Afl) and their spouses (Af2) as lower on overall affiliation than did the latter (3; = -4.63, p < .001; E = -3.08, p < .01, respectively). Hypothesis 3a. pertains to affectionate behavior in the marriage, without regard to any counterbalancing 120 ouoawuoo oocouuo> ououoooo an ouoeNuoo uocoNNo> ou~ooo 4 nucowau connouooocoz nucuaau odomouooocoz NNN. NN\H NN.- n> noooHHo ooNNoNNoo NNN. NN\H .NN.H- n> nocoHHo oonnouooo mansuoz onENoz NNN. NN\H NN.N- n; nuaoHHo connouooo NNN. NN\H .NN.N- n> nocoHHo NNNNNNNoo noun w up w unnuocoo noNN w up w unnuucoo mnemwuooeoo ones i m NN.NNN.NH N4 NoNNN NN.NNN.NH N4 NoNNN NNHN. NN.N NH.NNN.NN N nosouo coosuum HNNN. NN.NH NH.NNN.HHH N noaonm :oosuom noun M M as no ouusom coho M M me up mouaom cocoauo> uo namhaoad >o3oco NN.NNN NHnsNoz HN.HNN NHneNoz NN.NNH nucoHHo nonnoNNoocoz NN.NNH nucoHHo ounnouamocoz NN.NNH nucuHHo oonnonouo NN.NHH nucoHHo ooNNoNNoo coo: macaw coo: osouo unsoNN . coHunHHHNN< oouamHoz a NNN «Hon . :oHunHHHNNN oouamHm: . HNN :oNuoNNNuuc no unsouw no cemwuomeou vH manna 121 ouoawunu oocowuo> ououooon as uuofiwuoo oocoNuo> ooaoom a cascade ounnoumoocoz nucofiao ooonouooocoz No. mv\H «No.1 n> oucowao ooooouooo No. mv\H smH. i o> oucowau oonoouooo oaosuoz NHoEuoz «No. . NexH .NN.H- n> nuconHo ouunouooo NN. NN\H .NN.H- n> mucoNHo oonnunooo noNN w up w unnuucoo noun w up w unnuucoo nconwuooeoo once i m NNN.N N4 NoNNN NNNN . N4 NoNNN NNHH. on.~ NNN.¢N N edsoum cooauum mmmfl. oN.~ momN N nosoum coosuom noun M M o5 no ouusom noun M M we no oousom oocowuo> no naoxaoct hoaoco Nn.ocu oaoeuoz vm.ao~ oNoEuoz om.v- oucowao ooooouooocoz va.Hm~ avocado ooooouooocoz NN.NNN nucoHHo nonnonooo NN.NNN nucoHHo oonnououo sou: osouu coo: msouu cocoon . :oHunHHHNNN . Na NHoN . :oHuNHHHNNc . NNN :oNuoouud mo :oNoooummm co emaouo mo cauduomeoo mu manoh 122 ouoaauoo cocoNuo> ououonoo a. ouoENuno oocofluo> ooaoon a vm. mv\u a~o. nucowau oonuounoocoz n> oucoaau oonnounon nv. mvxn aoo.t oHoEuoz n> nucowao oonnuunon noun w up w. onnuucoo ocoowuonsoo none 1 M mam mv uouum NNNN . NNN . H NNN N 2?on H.333 noun M M we no uousom cocoauo> uo manhuocd nuance NH.NNm naoeuoz oo.von nusoauu ounmounoocoz NN.NoN oucowao ooooounuo coo: nsouo ouoom oaoom o>oq i mud coNuoouut no coNeoounwu co mnmouo mo coowuoneoo 0H manna :4 H. N a NED: :0 INQJNWNMO VG. 10am fih-XNEMNIUI AN AWNANnvsh 123 oQN. mN\H oueeauee cocowuo> ououonon as easuwdo oooneunoonoz ouoflwuoo oocouuo> oudoon a avocado ouonounoocoz NN.H as nucoHHo nonnouooo NNH. NN\H NN.H n> nucoHHo nonnouooo afloauoz voEuoz NNN. NN\H ..HN.N n> nucoHHo nonnouooo Noe. HNNH ..NH.N n> NHNNHHo oonnouooo nouo M up w unnuucoo noun w up w unnuucoo ocOmNuonEou once i m nvm mv uouum new we uouuu NNN. HN.N NNN.N N masons :oosoom NNNN. NN.N HNN.N N Nosoum coosuoo nOun M M 05 up ouusom noun M M as up oousom oocowuo> no owoaaosd unease HN.NH NHneNoz NN.N NHneuoz n o . on OHSOAHU UNQQOHQNGEOZ 0° . GN OUCOMHU QOQOOMQOUCOZ NH... nucuHHo ooNNoNNoo NN.N. NucoHHo connouooo coo! nsouu coo: nsouu ongoNN . NNHHHunoN . N: NHoN u NuHHHunoN . HN Nuwndueom so unmouo no cONNuonEoo ha manna 124 hostility. It states that depressed patients and their spouses express less affection and exchange fewer verbal and nonverbal loving behavior than do psychiatrically normal husbands and wives or other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. This hypothesis was tested in terms of SASB measures Af3 and Af4, Affiliation-Self and Affiliation- Spouse and Afs, the Love Scale Score. It was predicted that the Depressed Clients would score lower on all three measures than would either comparison group. These pre- dictions were not confirmed on measures Af3 and Af‘1 (see Table 15), the Depressed Clients differed from neither the Normals nor the Depressed Clients. The Depressed Clients rated their marital relationships no less favorably on the Love Scale than did either control group. As Table 16 indicates, neither between group comparison was statis- tically reliable. Hypothesis 3b. states that depressed clients and their spouses express more overt hostility and friction than do either normal husbands and wives or other psychotherapy clients and their spouses. This hypothesis was translated into the predictions that the Depressed Clients would score higher on SASB measures H -Hostility-Self and Hz-Hostility- 1 Spouse than would either comparison group. Two of the four predictions were confirmed. As Table 17 indicates, the Depressed Clients rated themselves as higher on hostility (H1) than did the Normals (E = 5.12, p < .05) but no dif- ferently than did the Nondepressed Clients (_t; = 1.57, p > 125 .10). The results were similar for measure H The 2. Depressed Clients rated their spouses as more hostile than did the Normals (E = 2.91, p < .01) but no more or less hostile than did the Nondepressed Clients (E = 1.06, p > .25). The fourth, and final, hypothesis of this study states that communication between depressed clients and their spouses is more impaired than that between either other therapy clients and their spouses or normal husbands and wives. To test this hypothesis the three groups were compared on the Marital Conununication Inventory. As Table 18 indicates the prediction that the Depressed Clients would score lower on this instrument was confirmed in part. The Depressed Clients' mean of 72.25 was significantly lower than the Normals' mean of 101.63 (E = -4.92, p < .001) but no different from the Nondepressed Clients' mean score of 82.25 (E = -1.67, p > .10). To provide more information about the relationship between severity of depression (rather than diagnostic category) and autonomy, dominance-submission, affiliation, hostility, and quality of communication, simple bivariate correlations (Pearson product moment) were computed between Beck Depression Inventory scores and scores on each of the 18 dependent measures. Table 19 presents these correla- tions. Fourteen of the 18 correlations were statistically reliable at the .05 level or higher. Only the measures of affiliative, loving behaviors, Af3, Af4, and AfS' and one 126 ounflquoo uocowuo> eunucnoo «a ouoawuno oocofiuo> ooaoon a HNH. NNNH .NN.H- nucoHHo nonnonooocoz n> nucoHHo oonnououo ooo. mvxn awo.vi odoEuoz o> nucoadu ounnounoo noun w up w unnuucoo nconwuoneou name I m mom mv uOuuu NNN. NN.NH NNN.N N Noaouo coosuom noun M. M as no oousom oocowuo> no nunaaosd nosoco no.HoH mnofiuoz m~.~o avocado commounoocoz m~.~N avocado ooooounoo coo: noouo No I oouoom not i soduoONGsaaoo so unwouo no :onauoneoo «a manna 127 goo. v Q «as HN. v N .. No. v N . ...HN.- Ham Ho COHUUUHCQEOU No mmhgmfimz ..NN. ...Nv. No.1 NH.u NH.- .NN.- ...No.u Hon N: H: Nun 4N: NN< Nun HNa :oNuoNHNuu4 no nauseous «one. eaaNm. eon. aeavo. aaewm. eeHv. n~.i eaahv.l 4400.1 aaamm.i Ham No: He: NN Hm No Ho No He NH HH cocooconoouuucu i haosouat no nauseou: cONuoofisaeEou one .coNuoanduucluamocound one :oNnoounon no auwuo>om i unoNuoaouuou on manna 128 measure of autonomy, Az-Friendly Autonomy-Spouse, were un- related to severity of depressive symptoms. In most instances, the correlations between. BDI scores and SASB ratings of one's own behavior were higher than those between BDI scores and SASB ratings of the spouse's behavior. As expected, the 18 dependent measures were also inter-correlated. Table 20 presents the correlation matrix for these measures. In several instances these correlations are quite high (.70). This suggests that the various E-test comparisons discussed above were providing redundant information. To reduce this redundancy a step-wise discrim- inant function analysis was conducted. This technique determined which subset of measures best discriminated between groups. The discriminant analysis reduced the space within which the participants could be plotted from the 18 dimensions defined by the dependent measures to a more wieldly and interpretable two dimensions. The number of dimensions is equivalent to number of groups-1. In this case the number of groups equahSthree, and thus the maximum number of discriminant functions that can be derived is two. Each dimension, or function, is a linear combination of some optimal subset of discriminating measures. Rao's V was chosen as the selection criterion for the step-wise analysis because this criterion maximizes overall separation between groups (Klecka, 1980, pp. 54-55). The computer' program employed was that of the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (2nd Edition) (Nie, Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner & Hooscaucoo canon. NN.- NN.- HN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- NN. NN. NN. NN. HNN NN. NN. NN. NN. NN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- N4: NN. NN. NN. NN. NN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- Ha: NN. NN. NN. NN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- NN NN. NN. NN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- Hm NN. HN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- No NN.- NN.- NN.- NN.- Ho 9 O. .. ... ... 2 1 NN. NN. NN.- N4 NN. NN. Ha HN. NH N Na. Na Na Na Na Na: He: NN HN No Ho Na Ha NH Hm HH nauseous ucoocenoo oH I xNuunz :oNuoHouuoo ON manna. om.l om.i ow. mo. no. no. mo. no.1 «v.1 o~.t mv.i no.1 no.1 oo. mm. mm. Nv. no. an. ov.i o~.1 no.1 «v.1 oo. nv. on. av. mo. mm. No.1 vn.1 0N.I «v.1 mn.l on.1 on.1 vo.i oo.| no.1 vo. on. on. No. oo. «v.1 nv.i om.i mm.1 130 oo. mm. mm. av. om.l nm.i oN. oo. mu.l o~.I Nv. mm. on.1 oH.i u< oo. on. em. vo.i mm.1 on. ma.1 om.l wH.I me. me. mu.l mu. u< cm. no. ov.| Ho.i Ha. no. on.1 on.1 am. No. no. ao. Nd am. No.1 no.1 mo.l on.l ou.i ev.i no. mm. ma. mm. w< v n N a N :1: H4 u¢ u< u‘ u< <3 4: ..@.u:OU. ON munch 131 Bent, 1975). This analysis used the SPSS default values for the selection criteria: tolerance, F-to-enter, and F-to- remove. [These criteria define the minimum conditions to be met for a variable's inclusion in a step-wise analysis.) Table 21 presents a summary of this analysis, which required only four steps. The four variables selected were Il-Weighted Autonomy-Self, AZ-Friendly Autonomy-Spouse, Af - 1 Weighted Affiliation-Self, and C -MCI Score. After these 1 variables were selected, none of the remaining variables contributed sufficient unique discriminating power to warrant inclusion on the functions. Two discriminant func- tions were derived, and of these, the first proved to be by far the more powerful disciminator. A comparison of the eigenvalues with Functions 1 and 2 reveals that Function 1 carried approximately 92% of the discriminating power in this system. As Klecka (1980, p. 36) emphasizes, however, the most powerful function derived in a discriminant analysis is not necessarily either (substantively) useful or (statistically) reliable in differentiating among groups. (After' all, a (discriminant analysis cannot. create group differences if the variables included in the functions are themselves weak in differentiating among groups.) Addi- tional measures must be employed to assess the usefulness and statistical significance of a discriminant function. A measure of a function's utility is its canonical correlation coefficient. This coefficient indicates the degree of relatedness between group membership and 132 NNHHN. V NNNNN.H oooov.i ovoom. Noomn. mova.Hl N :oNuucsn a cannonsn Hoowouucoo nsouo. mono: annouo no oouosao>m ocoNuocsn unocwafiuooao Hecacocou aoH. n Voo.m gun. a mm. oo.ooH NNH. «N coo. a o~m.~v mom. 0 no. em.um omn.~ ea UHm no Dflmdaom ¢DQZ ZOHBUZDE 1H=U mxAHz KNBhd ZOZ¢U :DU IZQOHN DCOMfiO—uflh UCUCMEHHONMO H ”Cacao—GU ooo.~ a 0>OBO¢ cu m Bflfiuxal ooo.H a usucm 0» N eoeHcN: Noo. u No>oq oocouoaoe seeded: > N.onz n ocean: ownawosc noduocon unusaaduoodo onasunuum mo auoessm an wanna 133 discriminant. function scores. One interpretation. of the canonical correlation which can be enlightening to those who are accustomed, as many psychologists are, to thinking in terms of the analysis of variance, is that the correlation is equivalent to eta. The canonical correlation squared is equivalent to eta-squared, that is, to the proportion of variation in discriminant function scores attributable to group membership (Klecka, 1980, p. 37). Judged by this criterion, only Function 1 is a good or useful discrimina- tor. Its canonical correlation of .76 is quite high; 57% of the variance in scores on this function can be explained by group membership. Function 2 proves to be far less useful in differentiating among participant groups. Its canonical correlation is an unimpressive .33. Only 11% of the variance in scores on this second function can be explained in terms of group membership. Because this. analysis includes sample, rather ‘than population, data, it is necessary to ask whether the group differences derived are "real" or merely the product of sampling error. That is, one must ask whether the dis- criminant functions are statistically significant. The most commonly used test of significance is Wilks' Lambda (Klecka, 1980, p. 38), which can be converted to a chi-square value. This derived chi-square value is, in turn, associated with a specific probability level. Wilks' Lambda is an indirect measure of significance in that it measures the residual discrimination in the system prior to deriving a function, 134 rather than the significance of the function itself. This is an inverse measure whose values range from zero to 1.0. A very small lambda indicates that a great deal of discrim- inating' power remains in the system (of ‘variables). A lambda which approaches the maximum value of 1.0 indicates that the remaining functions carry very little power to differentiate among groups. In this analysis .the Wilks' Lambda value prior to deriving the first function is .37. This indicates that the variables possess a fairly high degree of discriminating power. As Table 21 indicates, this lambda was converted to a chi-square value of 42.9, which in turn, is associated with a .000 probability level. This means that the between group differences are significant prior to extracting Function 1. .After’ deriving this first. function, Wilks' Lambda increased to .89. This indicates that little dis- criminating power remains in the system. The chi-square value of 5.00 derived from this Lambda is not statistically significant (p > .10). This means that the between group differences on Function 2 are not statistically significant. Consequently, in this discriminant analysis, the first function (extracted) emerged not only as the more powerful one, but as the only one which is both statistically sig- nificant and substantively useful. The second stage in assessing the results of a dis- criminant function analysis is the interpretation of the derived functions. Only Function 1 will be interpreted 135 because it alone proved substantively and statistically significant. In interpreting a function it is advisable to examine both the standardized (canonical) discriminant coefficients and the structure coefficients (Klecka, 1980, p. 34). The standardized discriminant coefficient indicates a variable's unique contribution to a discriminant function score. Thus this coefficient is affected by the simultane- ous contributions of all the other variables loading on that function. If two or more variables are intercorrelated, their standardized discriminant coefficients will be adjusted to correct for this redundancy. This correction can take the form of either decreasing the absolute value of both coefficients, or of assigning a negative sign to one of two equal-valued coefficients. The structure coefficient is a simple bivariate correlation between scores on a given variable and scores on the discriminant function. Unlike the standardized discriminant coefficient, it is unaffected by the simultaneous contributions of all the other vari- ables. As such, the structure coefficient is a simpler, more straightforward index of the relationship between a variable and a discriminant function. For these reasons, Klecka (1980, p. 34) recommends that the structure coeffi- cients be given greater weight in interpreting a discrimin- ant function. Table 22 presents both the standardized discriminant coefficients and the structure coefficients and recapitu- lates the correlation matrix for the four variables selected Hmoscwucoo manna. NNNN. NNNN. Na NNNN.- HNNN. HH N onNozoN H onNozoN mucoaoammoou ououoouum NNHNN. NNHNN. Ho % HNNHN. HNNNN. Hon 1 N NHNNN. NHNNN.- a emomm.i vomoo. HH N ZOHBUZDm H ZOHBUZDh mucoaoflmmoou :oNuocsn ucocNENuomNo Hooacocoo oonaouoocoum mflcmfl OHM“ 000 OHDHUQHUW UCM mucoaoammoou :oNuo:NENuomNo ooNNouoosoum mo somwuoneoo NN GHQMB 137 NNN.H NNNN. HNNN. NNNN. o NN.H NNNN. HNNN. HNa NNN.H NNNN. No NN.H Ho Ho Hun Na HH mamoaonm oma3inmum CH ooosaocH moanoaum> uooniixNuuoz :oNuoHouuou vvmo. mono. HO NNNN. NNHN. HNN N onNozoN H onNozoN mucowowmmooo ousuoouum H.o.ocooo NN oHone 138 by the step-wise analysis. The standardized discriminant coefficients are not easily interpreted when considered by themselves. Variables 11, A2, and C2 appear to be the most important in determining scores on Function 1. Yet I1- Weighted Autonomy-Self and Az-Friendly Autonomy-Spouse make opposite valenced contributions, and this is not easy to interpret. An examination of the structure coefficients suggests, however, that Function 1 is most closely related to 11' Afl, and C1. Their structure coefficients are .83, .72, and .73, respectively. That C1 correlates highly with Af1 and A2 (r = .78, .70, respectively) probably accounts for .Afl's lowered and .Az's larger, but negative, stan- dardized discriminant coefficients. Function 1 might then be best interpreted as a con- joint measure of overall autonomy, affiliation and communi- cation. One name which suggests itself for this function is "Affectionate Cooperation"; that is, the interpersonal orientation of one who is intimately involved with another without sacrificing his/her own identity or autonomy and who is able to adequately communicate his/her wants, needs and feelings to his/her partner. The third stage in assessing the results of a discrim- inant function analysis is classification. At this stage each case is classified according to its proximity to each of the group centroids. [A group centroid is that coordin- ate in n-dimensional space defined by the mean for that group on each of n discriminant functions.] A comparison of 139 the predicted classifications with actual group membership constitutes another test of the adequacy of the derived discriminant functions. In this analysis, a random assign- ment of cases to groups would, theoretically, result in a correct classification rate of 33%. The classification results should be evaluated in terms of this prior proba- bility. Tables 23 and 24 present the classification of cases according to discriminant scores. As Table 23 indicates, 75% of all the cases were correctly classified. Only two of the Depressed Clients were misclassified. Of the 16 Non- depressed Clients, three were assigned to the Depressed Clients group and two to the Normals. Five participants in the Normals group were assigned to the Nondepressed Clients group. These classification results are substantially better than chance, and attest to the adequacy and utility of the discriminant functions in differentiating among the three participant groups. Table 24 presents the classification of each of 48 cases. An a posteriori analysis of clinical and other back- ground information on the misclassified cases revealed no patterns. or’ characteristics which :might. account. for the misclassification. The two misclassified Depressed Clients presented no clinical or life history data which cast doubt on their original diagnosis or obviously differentiated them from the other Depressed Clients. Indeed, other Depressed Clients presented. more complex: and. difficult issues for 140 oo.mo I oowmammoHU oHuoouuoo momoo oonsouo mo unmouon o.oo m.Hm o mHoEuoz HH m o NH m nsouw m.~H o.oo o.oH madeHu oommounoocoz m HH M NH N noouo o m.~H mH.oo mucoHHO oommounoo o m «H oH H nsouu m N H momou «0 .oz noouo Hoouod anmuonEoz nsouo oouoaooun momoo no :oHuooHMHmmoHO mN manna n‘ei 141 Hmoscaucoo oHnou. NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NH-H NNNH. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NH-H NHHN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NH-H NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NH-H NNNH. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NH-H NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H HHuH NNHN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NH-H HNHH. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NIH HHNN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NIH NNNH. N NHNN. NNNN. H .H NuH NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H N-H NNNN. N HNNN. NNNN. H .H NuH NNNN. N NNHN. HNNN. N .44 .H 41H Nave. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H NIH NNNH. N NHNN. NNNN. N ... .H N-H NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. H .H HuH Hx\o. N Nsono Hx\oo N Ho\xo N ozono NsoNo Hnsuoa onno unmamHo onN NNHHHonnoNN unoamHo mouoom unocHEHuomHo on acaouoooa unsouo ou momoo mo ucmficmawmd om OHQMB 142 .mmscflucoo mHnmu. mmmv. N HHNm. mHVQ. m .m Hlm mmmm. m NMHw. v¢mm. N .N QHIN mev. m mmcm. Nmmm. N .N mHIN mmmH. m mmNm. «mmH. N .N vHIN mONv. m mmhm. mmNN. N .N mHlN mmNN. m momm. ahfim. N .N NHIN mNmN. N mbmo. Hwom. H ««« .N HHIN mmoH. m mmvm. mHvN. N .N oHlN ommm. H mwmm. mqu. N .N aIN momH. N hmmh. m¢H¢. m i%« .N mIN ¢Nmo. N Hhom. Homm. m «i« .N BIN vmhm. m Hhov. mev. N .N mIN HmHv. m vam. mmmm. N .N mIN NvHfi. N ommv. GOHO. H «fit .N le momH. N mHoh. mHNm. H ««« .N MIN wNmH. m mach. hHfim. N .N NIN mhmH. m vam. mmHN. N .N HIN .x\0. m macaw Ax\uv m Aw\x. m macho maouu Hmsuom ommu ummanm ocN NuHHHnmnoum ummzmHm A. c.u:oo. «N mHnma 142 .mmscwusoo mHnmu. NNNN. N HHNN. NHNN. N .N HuN NNNN. N NNHN. «NNN. N .N NHnN «NHH. N NNNN. NNNN. N .N NHuN NNNH. N NNNN. «NNH. N .N NHnN NoNN. N NNNN. NNNN. N .N NHuN NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. N .N NH-N mNmN. N mhmw. Hwom. H ««« .N HHIN NNoH. N NNNN. NHNN. N .N oHuN NNNN. H NNNN. NNHN. N .N NuN NomH. N NNNN. NNHN. N 1*. .N NsN «Nmo. N HNoN. HoNN. N N«« .N NuN «NNN. N HNoN. NHNN. N .N NIN HNHN. N vam. NNNN. N .N muN NvHv. N oqu. NoHo. H «N. .N qu ammH. N mHmh. mHNm. H «*1 .N MIN «NNH. N NNcN. NHNN. N .N NIN NNNH. N «HNN. NNHN. N .N H-N .x\u. m macaw Hx\o. m .w\x. m muouo msouo Hmsuoc mmmo ummamHm ocN NuHHHnmnoum ummgon 7%. HCOO. VN GHQMB 143 cwfimammmHomNE mmmo Nut umwnmumnEmE macho mo NHHHHnmnoum u .x\w. m NcHouuamo msoum scum Ham umnu mH mmmo NuHHHnmnoum u .u\x. m NNNN. N NNNN. NNNN. N *N« .N mHuN NNNN. N HONN. ammo. N .N NHuN NNNH. N NNNN. NNNN. N .N «HIN ONNN. N oovm. NNNN. N N*« .N NHuN NNOH. N NNNN. oHNN. N .N NHuN NHHH. N ONNN. Nose. N .N HHuN NNNo. N NHNN. NNON. N .N oHuN HNNH. N NNNN. NNNN. N .N NuN NONN. N NHNN. NNNN. N .N NuN «NON. N NNNN. ONNN. N .N NnN NNHN. N NONN. NNNN. N 4*; .N ouN mNNN. N NHNN. NNNN. N 44* .N NIN NHNN. N NNNN. «NNo. N .N NuN NNNo. N Hwom. NNHN. N .N NnN NoNH. N NNVN. NHNN. N *«« .N NuN Hx\o. m msouo .x\o. m .o\x. m maouo msouo Hmsuoa mmmu ummanm ocN NHHHHnmnonm ummanm ..fi.ucoov VN mHQMB a1 g1 144 their original diagnosis. The misclassified Nondepressed Clients, presented too varied clinical data and life- histories to suggest any reason for their misclassification. Two of the five misclassified Normal participants had reported mild psychological distress during the past two years, but the other three misclassified participants provided no evidence of emotional or marital difficulties. The most reasonable, or conservative, conclusion then is that the four variables selected in the step-wise analysis are good, but imperfect differentiators of the participant groups. CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION The guiding premise of this study is that clinical depression is a homeostatic mechanism in the marriages of depressed patients. One partner's symptoms, however dis- couraging, puzzling and vexing, may be nonetheless vital in maintaining the marital status quo. First, a depression maintains the status quo in that it, like other psychiatric syndromes, can preserve a marriage when it might otherwise disintegrate under the strain of environmental or matura- tional stressors. The depressed partner's demands for help, care, and sympathy deflect attention from the couple's basic or long-standing problems and conflicts. Secondly, clinical depression preserves the marital status quo in that its symptoms are congruent with, or even outgrowths of, the couple's relationship roles and rules. That one partner's depression prevents marital breakdown cannot be observed directly. Functional explanations are necessarily a matter of inference. Relationship roles and rules can, however, be reported on directly or inferred from the partners' habitual patterns of interaction. This study asked depressed out- patient therapy clients to report on numerous aspects of their relationships with their husbands and wives during the 145 146 three months prior to meeting with the principal investi- gator. This study hypothesized depression specific patterns of marital interaction. The clinical and research litera- ture to date suggest that the depressed patient and spouse form a depressogenic dyad which disallows autonomy and self-assertion, limits mature expression of affection, transforms disagreement or dissatisfaction into overt hostility, and prohibits open, straightforward communication between parties. This is a symbiotic bond in which the depressed partner's need to be cared for complements his/her spouse's need to be the caretaker. This is not an unam- bivalent relationship, however, for the excessive demands placed on the caretaker and the powerlessness and dependency felt by the identified patient give rise to mutual resent- ment and alienation as well. Moreover, these features of the marital relationship were proposed as specific to the depressed client, rather than as common to psychotherapy clients in general. Pre- sumably, the clinicians who have written about their depressed patients' family lives (e.g., Forrest, 1969; Hogan & Hogan, 1974; McLean et al., 1973; Rubinstein & Timmons, 1978) have (implicitly) compared these with the family relationships of their other clients. Yet such comparisons have not been discussed in their writings. Only a very few studies (notably Hautzinger et al., 1982) have systematically compared couples in which the identified patient was diagnosed as depressed with couples in which the 147 identified patient received some other psychiatric diag- nosis. Those systematic studies which have examined the family relationships of depressed patients (notably Hinchliffe, Hooper & Roberts, 1978; Weissman & Paykel, 1974) have relied on psychiatrically normal adults for control subjects. This study compared depressed outpatients, not only with symptom free adults, but also with other outpatient therapy clients, who might best be labelled as "garden variety neurotics." Because this latter group presented diverse clinical pictures and histories, no complex or symptom specific patterns of differences between the two clinical groups ‘were hypothesized. Rather, the principal investigator predicted simply that the differences between depressed outpatients and "normals" applied as well to comparisons with other therapy clients. The findings of this study provide considerable, but hardly unequivocal support for its hypotheses. First, the Depressed Clients differed from the control groups in rating their marriages on autonomy, dominance-submission, hostile autonomy, overall affiliation, hostility, and communication, but not on verbal and nonverbal expression of love and affection. In this regard the Depressed Clients evaluated themselves and their spouses no less positively than did any of the other participants. Secondly, on some measures significant differences were found only for the comparison between the Depressed Clients and the Normals but not on those between this target group and the Nondepressed 148 Clients. This latter limitation questions whether the hypothesized patterns of marital interaction characteristic of depressed outpatients and their spouses are in fact depression specific. Thirdly, the between groups difference on some variables were limited to the participants' self- ratings; these differences did not emerge on their ratings of their spouses. The results of this study suggest that it is in regard to autonomy and interdependence that the marriages of depressed persons differ most markedly from those of other psychotherapy clients or of symptom-free adults. On two SASB measures, Il-Weighted Autonomy-Self, and 141-Friendly Autonomy-Self, the Depressed Clients rated themselves as lower on asserting their own and promoting their spouses' autonomous identity and decision making than did both control groups. In rating their spouses on overall Autonomy and Friendly Autonomy (12 and A2) however, the Depressed Clients differed in the predicted direction from the Normals, but not from the Nondepressed Clients. This pattern of group differences is puzzling, for it suggests that the impairment in asserting and promoting autonomy in the marriage is depression specific for the identified patient, but not for his/her spouse. This is not to suggest that depressive symptomatology emerges and/or persists wholly independently of the "well" spouse's behavior. That the husbands and wives of the depressed out- patients were rated lower on autonomy than were the husbands 149 and wives of the psychiatrically normal participants sug- gests otherwise. One possible interpretation of these findings is that it is the depressed patient's failure to meet (or avoidance of) the demands of adult autonomy which is the more significant in the emergence of depressive symptoms. This retreat from autonomy, as some clinicians have suggested (notably Hogan & Hogan, 1975) may well have its source in prior significant family relationships. While the identified patient may play the dominant role in a relationship system that engenders depression; the ”well” spouse plays a significant collusive role as well. The comparisons of the participant groups on dominance and submissiveness yielded unexpected results. The study predicted that the Depressed Clients would rate themselves lower on dominance and higher on submission and their spouses higher on dominance and lower on submission than would any of the other participants in the study. On the SASB measures of dominance (D1 and D2), the Depressed Clients rated themselves and their spouses as exerting, or attempting to exert, greater interpersonal control in the relationship than did the Normals. On these measures the target group did not differ from the Nondepressed Clients control group. The Depressed Clients also described them- selves as more submissive (SASB measures 81) than did either control group. Surprisingly, the target group also des- cribed their spouses as more submissive than did the Normal 150 participants, though no differently than did the Non- depressed Clients. These results challenge the notion that the depressed patient reverts exclusively to the passive, dependent, child's role in his/her significant interpersonal relation- ships, while his/her partner assumes the role of all-power- ful parent. Instead, the evidence at hand suggests that the depressed client and spouse are in conflict over who shall direct the course of their life together. Apparently, the depressed patient's attempts at control extend beyond the passive manipulation of the depression itself. It must be noted though, that this increased dominance on the part of both client and spouse does not appear to be depression specific. The increased submissiveness of the Depressed Clients does though appear to be depression specific. There are two different, although not necessarily mutually exclusive, interpretations of the depressed client's increased submissiveness. The first, and perhaps more parsimonious account, is that the submissiveness is an aspect of the increased helplessness which is a symptom of depression. An alternative explanation is that the depressed partner's submissiveness is a pervasive feature of his/her marital relationship and that the overt helplessness of acute depression is an extension or outgrowth of this. These alternative explanations raise the difficult question of the direction of the casual link between submissiveness and clinical depression: Is submissiveness merely a symptom 151 or by-product of depression or is it a precondition for the emergence of clinical depression in the :marital system? Unfortunately, the evidence collected in this study cannot answer this question. An answer to this question requires information gathered both prior to and following, as well as during a client's depressive episode. One of the more surprising results of this study is that the Depressed Clients described their spouses as more submissive than did the Normal Controls. This challenges the notion that the "well" spouse is, or at least is per- ceived to be, the all-potent parent figure in the relation- ship. This increased submissiveness may be simply an attempt at appeasing or pacifying the unhappy spouse, whose symptoms may be taken as the reproach they in fact may be. A second possibility is that the "well" partner yields to and accommodates the identified patient because s/he too would wish, though unconsciously, to retreat from adult responsibility and the role of the caretaker. As several clinicians (Forrest, 1969; Rubinstein & Timmons, 1975) have noted, the marriageSof some depressed patients are shaped by an underlying conflict over who shall be the dependent, cared for one. The finding that the spouses of the Depressed Clients were rated as more submissive than the spouses of the Normal control subjects raises the intriguing question of whether and how this submissiveness might contribute to the identified. patient's} depression. One ;possible answer is 152 that the spouse's submissiveness reinforces an underlying marital agreement that this is a relationship based on the model of parent and child, that is, of mutual dependency. While the partners may battle over or exchange roles, the relationship's structure remains unchallenged. Autonomous action and decision making, which are essential features of adult-to-adult relationships, are avoided, if not actually prohibited in such a relationship. The "well" partner's submissiveness may serve to remind the depressed partner that any fundamental change in the relationship rules is impossible. The relationship roles and rules of depressed out- patients and their spouses may limit or even prohibit each partner's autonomy, but they do not ensure a harmonious, uninterrupted symbiosis. On the contrary, the Depressed Client's ratings indicate that their marriages are punctu- ated by considerable, mutual alienation. These participants rated themselves as more likely to withdraw from or dismiss their spouses in times of anger and hurt than did either the Nondepressed Clients or Normal participants. The Depressed Clients also rated their spouses higher on hostile autonomy than did the Normals, but no differently than the Non- depressed Clients. Thus, in the depressed patient's marriage autonomy apparently assumes the negative, even degenerate, form of alienation. There are (at least) two interpretations of the association between hostile autonomy and clinical 153 depression. First, mutual alienation may be a response to the depression itself. The depressed spouse withdraws in anger from a partner who cannot or will not meet his/her exaggerated, and often inarticulate, demands for help, care and reassurance. Similarly, the "well" partner becomes frustrated and angered by his/her spouse's demands and walls him/herself off. An alternative interpretation is that hostile autonomy is a precursor and reinforcer, as well as a concomitant, of depressive symptomatology. A relationship system which disallows mature, straightforward expression of needs and wants and which constrains husbands and wife to (assume) either the child's or the parent's role can grow unbearable for both partners. Such a system allows for little self-correction. Rather, the only relief from its constraints, the only means of protest the system allows is flight and isolation. The "well” partner retreats from the identified patient's dependency and/or dominance and thereby precipitates, or at least reinforces, the partner's feelings of despair and helplessness. Conversely, the "sick" partner may retreat in protest from his/her spouse's domination and/ or failure to meet his/her dependency needs. Angry with- drawal not only precludes more effective communication, which might resolve basic marital conflicts and misunder- standings, but also intensifies the depressed partner's feelings of helplessness, sadness, and futility. The evidence collected in this study cannot by itself confirm either of these interpretations. More extensive data, 154 gathered both prior to and following depressive episodes is needed to determine the temporal, and ostensibly causal, connections between hostile autonomy and depression. It is with respect to the expression of affection and of hostility that the marriages of the depressed outpatients differed least from those of the control subjects. The results of this study provide little support for its hypotheses concerning these variables. The findings fail to confirm the key hypothesis that the depressed patient inhabits an affectionless and emotionally unrewarding family environment. On the three measures of affiliation and verbal and nonverbal loving behaviors, Af3, Af4, and Afs, the Depressed Clients' ratings of themselves and of their spouses were no lower than those of either the Normals or the Nondepressed Clients. The Depressed Clients reported that they and their spouses exchanged as many verbal and nonverbal tokens of affection, offered each other as much help and support, and welcomed physical/sexual contact as muchx as did. the participants who reported no emotional distress or psychiatric symptoms. That the Depressed Clients scored no lower on the Love Scale (Afs) than the Normals control group is particularly noteworthy, because this scale appears to be a good index of generalized marital dysfunction and unhappiness. Research on the Love Scale has shown that scores on it are signifi- cantly correlated with other measures of marital satisfac- tion (Swensen, 1978) and are significantly lower among 155 couples seeking' professional. marital. counseling (Fiore & Swensen, 1977). Thus, the target group's "normal" scores on this measure suggest that the depressed patient's assessment of his/her marriage involves more than the commonplace com- plaints of discord and unhappiness. This is not to suggest that the depressed patient's marriage is entirely happy, adaptive or growth promoting. The Depressed Clients' high scores on hostile autonomy and hostility indicate otherwise. Rather, this finding of "normal" levels of affiliation and affectionate exchange in the marriages of the Depressed Clients suggests that what- ever is amiss in their marriages lies beyond such overt issues as whether these couples are physically affectionate, whether they assist each other with household chores or job- related tasks, or whether they remember each other's birth- days. Several clinicians have observed that the marriages of their depressed. patients may appear to be quite happy. Rubinstein and Timmons (1978) have suggested that the bond between the depressed patient and spouse as overclose and overwarm. The Bristol research group (Hinchliffe, Hooper & Roberts, 1978) has observed that the marriages of (many of) their depressed patients are superficially adequate and supportive. On the surface there are few signs of conflict, but on a deeper level the interaction is quite negatively toned. 156 The mutual affection and concern reported by the Depressed Clients did not, however, preclude reports of high levels of hostility in their marriages. This target group rated themselves (H1) and their spouses (H2) as higher on hostility than did the Normals control subjects. This find- ing accords with previous findings of higher than ”normal" levels of overt friction between depressed women and their husbands (Weissman & Paykel, 1974), of increased levels of marital conflict during and after depressive episodes (Rounsaville et al., 1979, 1980), and of depression related increases in overall hostility level (Fava et al., 1982). The temporal and causal connections between hostility and clinical depression, however, remain undetermined. The hostility the Depressed Clients reported may be simply a reaction to the depression itself. The ”sick" partner's anger may be little more than an expression of frustration and disappointment with a spouse who can neither alleviate his/her depressed mood nor restore his/her self-esteem and self-confidence. Similarly the "well" spouse's anger may simply reflect his/her frustration and resentment in the face of the depressed partner's constant and strident demands for help and reassurance. An alternative account of the depressed couple's hostility is that, as the Yale researchers have suggested (Rounsaville et al., 1979, 1980; Weissman & Paykel, 1974), it is a pervasive and enduring feature of the marriage. While the hostility may intensity during symptomatic 157 episodes, conflict and resentment both antedate and persist after the acute depressive periods. Both partners are angered by a relationship system which perpetuates mutual dependency, but. which fails to satisfy their dependency needs. Moreover, increased hostility and marital conflict have not been shown to be depression specific. The studies previously cited (e.g., Fava et al., 1982; Rounsaville et al., 1979, 1980; Weissman & Paykel, 1974) did not compare depressed patients with other diagnostic groups. In this study, the Depressed Clients reported no higher levels of hostility than did the Nondepressed Clients. Considered conjointly, the findings pertaining to affectionate behavior and. to hostility suggest that the marriages of depressed outpatients are not so much loveless as ambivalent. These results can also explain. why the Depressed Clients scored lower on the two measures of overall affiliation (Afl-Weighted Affiliation-Self and Afz- Weighted Affiliation-Spouse). These two SASB scores take into account a subject's ratings on all 72 items on a sur- face. These include ratings of both affiliative and hostile behaviors. The between group differences on these measures may thus be explained as a function of the Depressed Clients group's high scores on hostility (H1 and H2) and hostile autonomy (HA1 and HA2), rather than of their ratings on affiliation and affection. 158 The results of this study support the hypothesis that communication between the depressed patient and his/her spouse is impaired. The Depressed Clients scored signifi- cantly lower on the Marital Conununication Inventory (MCI) than did the Normal control group. The mean MCI score of this target group was 72.25, which is 1.75 standard devia- tions below the mean score of 105 for the normative samples on which this instrument was developed and validated (Bienvenu, 1970, 1978). The mean score of the Depressed Clients is also comparable to the mean score of 79.56 for the group of 16 depressed female inpatients studied by Wasli (1977). This impairment in communication cannot, however, be said to be depression specific because the difference in MCI scores of the two client groups was not statistically sig- nificant. The failure to find. a significant difference between the Depressed and Nondepressed client groups may be a function of the questionnaire used. The Marital Communi- cation Inventory can hardly be described as a complex instrument which probes for qualifications, nuances, and inconsistencies between message and Emma-message levels of communication. Rather, its straightforward questions ask whether husband and wife confide in each other or refrain from discussing problems and feelings, whether they argue over money matters or can disagree without losing control of their tempers. It does not ask precisely how, or with which words, husband and wife make requests or demands of each 159 other, or register complaints, whether and how long partners will discuss a problem until they reach a solution, whether and when humor or anger enters a conversation or precisely how each partner responds to criticism. The Marital Communication Inventory, thus, provides only a first-line indictation of impaired communication. Yet, on further reflection it may be that the critical question is not whether impaired communication is depression specific, but rather, which aspects of impaired communica- tion are depression specific. If as family-systems theorists contend (e.g., Jackson, 1968) psychiatric symptoms preserve relationship roles and rules, and such roles and rules are embodied in patterns of communication, then, one might expect to find faulty communication patterns in the marital or family units of any (diagnostic) group of psycho- therapy clients. Recent studies by the Bristol group (Hinchliffe et al., 1978) and by Hautzinger and colleagues (Hautzinger, Linden & Hoffman, 1982) have attempted more complex and sophisticated analyses of the communication processes between depressed patients and their spouses. Only the latter study compared depressed client couples with a psychiatric control group, and it did reveal depression specific impairments in marital communication. The findings of the present study indicate that some- thing is indeed. amiss in the patterns of communication between depressed clients and their husbands and wives. The Depressed Clients group reported greater reticence, tension 160 and friction on the part of both spouses than did the symptomrfree participants. What is needed. now is addi- tional, detailed information about which topics of conversa- tion are broached and which are avoided, and how and when tension and friction are expressed and how these are resolved or dissipated, etc. Such information may illumin- ate whether and how impaired communication contributes to the identified patient's depression. This study compared its three participant groups on multiple measures of autonomy, dominance-submission, affiliation-affection, hostility and quality of communica- tion, as if these variables were independent of each other. While these are theoretically distinct, they in fact appear to be related to each other. The correlations among the 18 dependent measures indicate, for example, that quality of marital communication correlates quite highly with friendly autonomy (.65, .70) and overall affiliation (.78, .82) and negatively with hostile autonomy (-.62, -.73) (see Table 20). A posteriori, these correlations are hardly puzzling. It seems most plausible that partners who communicate well are those who are warm and close with each other and who are self-assertive, independent, and noncontrolling. Yet the strength of these associations could not necessarily have been predicted a priori. Moreover, these correlations indicate that the between groups comparisons on all 18 dependent measures entailed a fair amount of redundancy. 161 The step-wise discriminant function analysis revealed that a linear combination of three SASB measures, Weighted Autonomy-Self, Friendly Autonomy-Spouse, and Weighted Affiliation-Self plus the Marital Communication Inventory Score, performed best in differentiating among the three groups. The name of this combination which suggests itself to the principal investigator is ”Affectionate Cooperation" because cooperation would seem to be the interpersonal stance of one who maintains his identity and autonomy in a loving, intimate relationship and who communicates effec- tively with his partner. While it is tempting to suggest that it is precisely a deficiency of "Affectionate Coopera- tion" which distinguished the Depressed Clients from the two control groups in the study, some caution is in order. The inclusion criterion used in the step-wise analysis, Rao's V, is one which maximizes overall group separation, rather than separation between one group and all the others. Moreover, the one statistically significant discriminant function derived in this analysis performed well, but imperfectly, in differentiating among groups. The between groups overlap on discriminant function scores was large enough to misclassify 12 of the 48 cases. There are two competing explanations to account for these misclassifications. The first, and more problematic pos- sibility, is that these 12 participant cases were initially miscategorized. The alternative account is that the initial categorization of cases was essentially correct, but that 162 the dependent measures, and consequently the discriminate function derived from these, are at best imperfect in differentiating among groups. Five of the apparently symptom-free psychologically normal participants were classified with the Nondepressed Clients. This may not be so surprising or puzzling on further consideration. One possible reason for these mis- classifications is that seemingly' healthy ‘volunteers *who agree to participate in a psychologist's study of marriage are more likely to be concerned about or dissatisfied with their marriages. They may be concerned about aspects of their marriages which may well prove to be the source of future conflicts and/or symptoms. A second possibility is that any sample of the "normal" adult married population, will include a subset whose marital interaction pattern resembles those of a clinical population. Such similarity, though, entails) no conclusions about risk; of subsequent emotional or psychiatric disturbance. The former explana- tion suggests that, in. effect, five "normal" volunteers miscategorized themselves. The latter explanation implies that certain patterns of marital interaction, although more prevalent in a clinical population, are hardly unique to that group. The evidence gathered in this study cannot eliminate either alternative. [The relatively high per- centage of misclassified cases (five of 16) suggest to this author that the first explanation is the more probable.] 163 The misclassification of the client participants raises more difficult issues. These participants had been interviewed and assessed by two clinicians, yet their classifications by discriminant function scores placed two Depressed Clients in the Nondepressed Clients groups and three Nondepressed Clients in the Depressed Clients group and two in the Normals group. These misclassifications may be attributed to initial miscategorizations. Such diagnos- tic errors might be due either to limitations in the principal investigator's skill and/or her access to pertinent diagnostic information or to inadequacies of the diagnostic guidelines employed. The possibility of diagnostic error is mitigated by several considerations. First, an a posteriori review of all the participant cases revealed no systematic differences between those participants who were correctly and those who were incorrectly classified. Secondly, several of the correctly classified clinical cases had initially posed diagnostic difficulties for the principal investigator and the referring clinician. Thus it is dubious that the mis- classified cases were precisely those that had been miscate- gorized to begin with, although the available evidence cannot disprove this possibility. An alternative, and more plausible, explanation for the misclassification of cases is that the original assign- ment of cases to groups was essentially correct, but that the 18 discriminating variables are imperfect group 164 differentiators. A classification of cases based on dis- criminant function scores derived from these measures would then be expected to include some errors. The Depressed Clients differed from the Normals on all but three of the 18 dependent measures, but on only four measures (II-Weighted Autonomy-Self, Al-Friendly Autonomy-Self, Sl-Submissiveness- Self and KAI-Hostile Autonomy-Self) did this target group differ from the Nondepressed Clients as well. The misclas- sification of at least a few client cases is thus not surprising in light of this pattern of group differences. The findings that the Depressed Clients differed from the Nondepressed Clients on only four dependent measures and that seven of the 32 client participants were classified in the wrong client group challenge this study's basic hypothesis that the marital interaction patterns character- istic of depressed psychotherapy clients and their spouses are depression specific. The author hypothesized that the marriages of de- pressed outpatients are uniquely characterized by decreased autonomy, increased hostile autonomy, decreased affection, increased hostility, and impaired communication. Not only were the Depressed Clients' ratings of affectionate, loving behaviors no lower than those of either control group, but their ratings of dominance, hostility and impaired communi- cation differed only from those of the Normals group. Yet, these latter dependent. measures correlated significantly with scores on the Beck Depression Inventory. All but four 165 dependent measures, three of affiliation (Af3, Af4, Afs) and one of the spouse's Friendly Autonomy (A2) correlated significantly with this measure of severity of depression (see Table 19), although for only four measures (11, Afl, S1 and. HA1) were the between. group) differences depression- specific. These seemingly inconsistent findings can, how- ever, be reconciled. The mere presence of depressive symptoms does not by itself specify a diagnosis of primary unipolar depression. On the contrary, depressive symptoms accompany a variety of psychiatric and medical disorders (Beck, 1972, p. 73). Consequently, a statistically significant, but. moderate, correlation between symptom severity and some other variable does not. entail a significant relationship» between that variable and the diagnosis of primary, unipolar depression. Many of the Nondepressed Client participants, it will be recalled, reported low mood and other symptoms of depres- sion. These participants also reported above normal levels of hostility and of dominance as well as impaired communica- tion in their marriages. Thus just as depressive symptoms appear to be common to a wide range of therapy clients, so too increased dominance and hostility and impaired communi- cation appear to be common to their marriages as well, regardless of primary diagnosis. These characteristics of marital interaction appear to be associated with the presence of depressive symptoms, but not with a diagnosis of primary depression. Aui Sel De} th. hie Th. di; of do: 5‘11 ma: a1. de] fi: 0v. the ti< cl: anc PEI dev eVi tha 166 On the other hand, four dependent measures, Weighted Autonomy-Self, Friendly Autonomy-Self, Hostile Autonomy- Self, and Submissiveness-Self} did. differentiate the Depressed from the Nondepressed Clients participating in this study. These measures were also those which correlated highest with the Beck Depression Inventory (see Table 19). This suggests that it is precisely these variables which differentiate psychotherapy clients who report some symptoms of depression from those whose current clinical picture is dominated by more pronounced and pervasive depressive symptomatology. The finding that it is the dependency relationship, marked by lowered autonomy and increased submissiveness and alienation which uniquely characterizes the marriages of depressed psychotherapy clients concurs with the research findings and clinical observations of several other authors. Overall (1971), for example, working from data gathered from a very large sample of psychiatric patients, has concluded that depression is particularly related to dependency rela- tionships in less than adequate social settings. Several clinical writers, notably the Hogans (Hogan & Hogan, 1975) and Rubinstein and Timmons (1978) have observed that the two person dependency relationship is the core feature of the depressed patient's family system. The third limitation on the extent to which the evidence gathered in this study confirms its hypotheses is that the depression specific between group differences di be re def ab: par dim The is alt ext 167 emerged only for the participants' self-ratings. On none of the dependent measures were the comparisons between the two client groups' ratings of their spouses statistically sig- nificant. The Depressed Clients rated their spouses less favorably on Weighted Autonomy (I2), Friendly Autonomy (A2), Dominance (DZ), Submissiveness ($2), Hostile Autonomy (HA2), weighted Affiliation (Afz), and Hostility (H2) than did the Normals, but no differently than did the Nondepressed Clients. This suggests that the husbands and. wives of depressed outpatients, do not behave, or are not perceived to behave, as "normally" or desirably as the husbands and wives of psychiatrically normal, symptom-free adults. On the other hand, this target group of spouses appears not to differ from the spouses of other psychotherapy clients. Two different, but not necessarily mutually contra- dictory, interpretations. can. account. for' the udiscrepancy between the Depressed Clients' self-ratings and their ratings of their spouses. One interpretation is that the depressed partner plays the dominant role in defining this abnormal marital relationship while the "well" spouse plays a secondary but necessary role as well. That the depressed partner assumes the dependent, submissive role in no way diminishes his/her power in defining the marital system. The identified patient's critical role in defining this bond is thus reflected in his/her extreme self-ratings. An alternative explanation is that the depressed patient's extreme, negative self-ratings on autonomy (11, A HA1) and 1’ on is as 5? pa as ex de de st‘ De co pr th pr ru‘ de di (I) the Cav tio Con int Fri.J 168 on submissiveness (SI), are but expressions of the negativ- ism, self-blame, and sense of powerlessness which are aspects of the depressive experience itself. The former interpretation suggests that the well spouse's role in the emergence and maintenance of his/her partner's depression is less important than this study has assumed. This study has presupposed, although it has not explicitly hypothesized, that the two members of the depressive dyad contribute equally to a unique pattern of depressogenic interactions. The evidence gathered in this study does not support this view. The latter account of the Depressed. Clients' extreme self-ratings, if' it .alone is correct, more seriously undermines a family systems inter- pretation of clinical depression. Such an account denies that depression is a homeostatic mechanism, is both the product and perpetuator of a couple's basic relationship rules. Again, the data gathered. here is inadequate to decide between these two interpretations. Two very important caveats must be included in the discussion of this study's findings. The first is that these findings are of limited generalizability. The second caveat is that, the author's obvious theoretical predelic- tions notwithstanding, the study's findings do not constitute proof of a causal relationship between marital interaction patterns and the emergence and maintenance of primary depression. 169 The findings of this study are of limited generaliza- bility because the principal investigator was able to interview and query the identified patient partner only once during the course of his/her treatment. Moreover, the questionnaires employed asked a participant to assess his/ her own and his/her spouse's behavior for only the three months period prior to meeting with the principal investiga- tor. The limitations of such a data base are obvious. First, the inferences drawn from this data. apply, strictly speaking, to the participants' perception of their marital interaction, rather than to the behavior itself. This is particularly true of inferences about the partici- pants' spouses. These inferences are based on second—hand reports. Had these spouses all rated themselves and their partners, a different, although not necessarily more honest, accurate or objective, picture of their marriages might have emerged as well. Thus, strictly speaking, one cannot generalize from the participants' perceptions of their own and their spouses' behavior to the behavior itself. Yet information about perceptions of interpersonal interaction is certainly not without value, for such perceptions no doubt influence overt behavior and emotion. A second limitation on the generalizability of these findings is that they pertain only to the three month period prior to participation in the study. Whether the patterns of interaction characteristic of the weeks including the onset of symptoms is characteristic of the depressed 170 patient's marriage in general cannot be answered by this study. It can be argued that these marital interaction patterns are specific to the depressive period. Two com- ments are in order. First, it is assumed that the patterns of increased. dominance and submission, hostile autonomy, hostility, and of impaired communication are more pronounced during clinical episodes. Secondly, other research evidence indicates that these features of the marital relationship are not unique to the symptomatic period. Weissman and Paykel's (1974) study revealed persistent overall impairment in the depressed women's performance in the marital role up to one full year after symptomatic recovery. These researchers also noted that at least half of the marriages of the 27 depressed women who were living with their husbands to be chronically difficult and unstable prior to symptom onset. Additional reports from the Yale depression research group (Rounsaville et al., 1979, 1980) indicate that conflicts between depressed women and their husbands persisted for months, and in some cases, even years after symptomatic recovery. The Bristol research group (Hinchliffe et al., 1978) has also reported that the highly negatively toned quality of the communication between acutely depressed women and their spouses was evident at symptomatic recovery as well. The reports on the marital and family treatment of depression (see Feldman, 1976; Forrest, 1969; McLean et al., 1973; Rubinstein & Timmons, 1978) also imply that the excessive interdependency and its 171 concomitant impairment in communication and affect exchange is an enduring characteristic of the depressed patient's intimate relationships. The third limitation on the generalizability of these findings is that they apply only to outpatients in psycho- therapy. The patterns of marital interaction documented in this study may not apply to all people experiencing clinical depression. First, the patterns of interdependency, hostility and communication may differ for couples in which one partner's depression is so severe as to warrant hospi- talization. For example, as Forrest (1969) has observed, the marital interaction of these inpatients may be virtually devoid of overt friction and hostility. Secondly, not all people who become clinically depressed seek psychotherapy. It may be that their marriages differ from those of depressed psychotherapy clients. Indeed, the possibility that anyone seeking such therapy is more likely to describe his/her marital and family relationships in highly negative terms (in order to justify his need of help) cannot be as yet ruled out. The second critical caveat in interpreting the evidence collected and presented here is that it provides no proof of a causal connection between marital interaction and clinical depression. This data pertains only to the client participants' marriages during a three month period which in many cases included symptom onset. Such limited data cannot specify which came first, the abnormal marital interaction 172 patterns or the symptoms of depression, and thus cannot demonstrate causal connections. Unfortunately the practical realities of research with a clinical population makes it exceedingly difficult to establish conclusively the temporal, and thus presumably, causal, connections between marital and family processes and symptom onset. Ideally, a research project would interview and query clients both prior to symptom onset and following sympto- matic recovery, as well as during the acute symptomatic phase. Yet researchers cannot easily identify and query clients before they first become symptomatic. While it is preferable to interview clients as early in the treatment process as possible, even data gathered then must be inter- preted with caution. Retrospective accounts of marital and family processes gathered at intake interviews may be affected by the client's having been symptomatic for weeks before requesting help. Data gathered after symptomatic recovery must also be interpreted judiciously. Marital interaction patterns following recovery are not necessarily identical to those of the premorbid period. One cannot assume that. a marriage remains unaltered by the couple's experience with the identified patient's depression. This experience may have permanently altered each partner's per- ception of him/herself, his/her spouse and/or the marriage. Long-standing conflicts and resentments may have been per- manently exacerbated by the identified patient's sickness. Thus, even extensive data gathering, while necessary and 173 desirable, may not be able to demonstrate conclusively the temporal connection between interpersonal processes and the onset and maintenance of clinical depression. This limitation which seems to be unavoidable in many clinical research projects, poses a critical problem for the researchers who adhere to a linear model of causality. If, on the other hand, one assumes, as a family systems-frame- work does, that causal connections within family processes are circular, rather than linear (von Bertalanffy, 1974), the question of which phenomenon precedes which, the inter- personal interaction process or the emergence of symptoms, diminishes in importance. This is not to suggest that a family systems conceptualization needs only limited data for confirmation. On the contrary, a family systems researcher needs as extensive information about family and .marital roles and rules preceding, during and following a clinical depression as does any other researcher. Yet a family-systems framework, which employs such concepts as deviation amplifying feedback mechanisms (Wender, 1968), can more easily accommodate data in which the temporal sequences of interpersonal process and symptom onset remain imprecise, for such a theoretical framework assumes that these variables are mutually causative. If one assumes that interpersonal processes can give rise to psychiatric symptoms, which in turn influence those processes; then some degree of uncertainty as to which occurred first is far less intellectually troubling than if 174 one assumes that causal connections are linear and intransi- tive. Admittedly, all that this study has demonstrated is the co-occurrence of depressive symptomatology with certain abnormal features of marital interaction which has been proposed as congruent with the symptoms themselves. Yet this evidence can be construed as preliminary additional systematic support for a family—systems conceptualization of depression. Appendices 175 APPENDIX A Target Groups Clients 1. Age - 20-60 years 2. Currently married (or cohabitating) and not separated extensively during past three months. Depressed Clients Nondepressed Clients A client will be included A client will be included in this group if he/she: in this group if he/she: a. Receives a diagnosis of a. Presents symptoms/com— of primary, unipolar de- plaints associated with pression agreed on by two one of the following clinicians. diagnoses: anxiety neu- rosis, phobic neurosis, b. Scores 18 or higher on obsessive-compulsive the Beck Depression neurosis, hysteria, sex- Inventory. ual disorder, character disorder, schizophrenia c. Has not received shock in remission, borderline therapy in past six personality organization months. b. Does not receive d. Is not psychotic. the diagnosis: primary unipolar or bipolar de- pression, alcoholism/ alcOhol abuse, organic brain syndrome, mental retardation. c. Does not score above 13 on the Beck Depression Inventory. d. Has not received shock therapy in past six months. ‘ e. Is not psychotic. 1. S. 176 APPENDIX B Guidelines for Briefing Client Participants Inform about the research project: Subject/purpose: To learn more about the marriage relationship of men and women who have been exper- iencing problems similar to the client's. Principal Investigator: Paula C. McNitt of Charleston, Illinois (if client seems concerned about credentials, mention that McNitt, an exper- ienced psychologist, is a doctoral candidate at Michigan State University, who has worked at men- tal health facilities in East Central Illinois). Invite/urge client to participate: Explain that client's participation will provide the mental health center with useful information which may help in the client's treatment. Clients will be paid $5.00 as a “Thank-you" for their participation. Outline what participation will involve: a. b. Client will be interviewed by Ms. McNitt. Inter view will last up to one hour. Husband/wife may (if willing and able) be inter viewed by Ms. McNitt. Interview will last about one hour. Client (and spouse optional) will complete several questionnaires. This should take about one hour. Reassure clients that all information they provide will be kept in strictest confidence. Offer feedback session in which questionnaire answers will be reviewed and interpreted. 177 APPENDIX C Diagnosing Primary Unipolar Depression This is less iaposing than it sounds. basically. this diagnosis applies to clients the are depressed and for whoa depression is the prison 'fs'échiatric gates; rather the an adjunct to seen other pro-existing psyc atr c t sue as schisophrenia or alooholisa. The following diagnostic criteria will be used (Note parallel structure in .. ,_ M of Diagnonic lnforntion) . 1 A. Dyaphoric Ibod I Sad, blue, discouraged. hopeless, irritable. fearful. wor- ri I. At least four of the following: l. Poor appetite or weight loss (or gain)‘ 2. Sleep difficulty - insomia, hyperso-Iia. or early nrning wehoning 3. boss of energy, fatigability or tiredness d. Agitation: or retardation 5. loss of interest in usual activities or decreased libido 6. Feelings of self-blaao, self-reproach or guilt 1. Oleaints of or actually diainished ability to think or concentrate I. Recurrent thoughts of death or suicide. including thoughts of wishing to be dead c. Depressive syntoas lasting at least 2 to 3 weeks with no pro-existing psychiatric condition such as: 1. Anxiety Msisu‘l'his refers to chronic free-floating anxiety with recto-rent anxiety attacks. Ibst dipressed clients report anxiety. ad soaetiaos experience anxiety prior to the onset of a depression, as if to fight off the letter. If, however. the anxiety symptoms are long standing and doainste the clinical picture, assess as anxiety nemesis rather than priaary depression. lf anxiety merely aceoqenies or fore. shadows the depression. assess as priaary depression. 2. Phobic Mair-This refers to clear—cut phobias of the type described in textbooks. 3. steria (This is a sore difficult diamosis.)--‘l'his refers to comer- s on syaptoas and fugue states. as well as chronic. vague aultiple mox- plained aedical coaplaints. (Use clinical judgnent here). Note: A priaary depression say be seen in clients shose personality style night be called "hysterical", i.e. i-aturity. labile aoods. psmdohypersexu- ality. etc. . d. Obsessive-C lsive Neurosis-J'his refers to obsessive thoughts and fantasies an: I(go—spills!“ behaviors. rituals. and 12; to the obsessive- coapulsive neurotic personality style. 178 S. Alcoholism/Alcohol Abuseu‘lhis refers to the condition as -described in nst textEEEs. He—evy drinking nay accoqaany depression, especially in .aen. lf increased drinking is specific to the depressed period and does not overshadow the latter, assess as priaery depression. if it is too difficult to decide whether the drinking or the depression is the pri- aary condition, diagnose the depression as figgog. 6. Waffle refers to dependency on street drugs or abuse of prescript on rugs Idlich antedates the depressive syaptoas. 7. Antisocial Personalit «Disgrace only if strong evidence of chronic delmcy, antisocial behavior. vagrancy. myocational and/or nrital asladjustaent. d. Schismreniao-Assess by either presenting smtoas, or history. by so -report or clinical record o_r_ by aedicstion history. Present or past use of any of the aajor tranquilizers, such rs Navane, Hellaril, or Thorotine. should be taken as prise facie evidence of schizophrenia. 9. Sense! Deviations-Jhis refers to deviations such as transennlity, or seams ty rs r than to semi dysfmction. 10. m1: Drain SM 11 . 'hental Retardation 12. Life Threat-ling lllness D. No evidence of Mic Episodes--Assess in terns of history or of lithiu- treatasnt. I. A i’ll-yt'nt-ultl tn.tlc sought p~ytltinttic as- sisnmcc dming tltc wintct quarter of his tltitd ycatt itt .1 highly cmnpctititc cnllcgc. Hc was of supctinr intelligence and tinting thc . pt etc-ding two years had «asked hard enough to bring his cumulative grade point average f t otn C-minus to a solid 3 average. However. he had not found much satis- faction in his work and was currently ex. pt “sing uncertainty regarding his voca~ tional goals. He was quite inltutct'tcd. had nor participated itt many athletic or social actititics. and tended to become deeply in- voltcd in relationships with a few other students of both sexes and to lit-come disap- points-d when they failed to reciprocate or liyc up to his expectations. . The deptt-ssion that led to his seeking psychmhcrapy had been pt ccipitntcd by the loss of his current girl friend. but he did on report this immediately. His presenting complaints were as follows. "l’vc been de- pressed -just different things bothered me. My relation to my parents I can't be free of them. and my ideas are very different front theirs. And I have a strong fear of social gatherings. and inability to make friends. And my home life was very unhappy. My parents have net been happy. it's been more like a battleground than a home. The depres- sion comes and goes. Sometimes I feel I don't want to do anything. Sometimes I go for months thinking that the world doesn't make sense. lf my feelings didn't change 1 would't want to go on living. In high school I remember feeling miserable because my parents naggt-d me. In the seventh grade I wished I could put aside the next ten years and live my own life.“ 179 APPENDIX D Sample Case His tories A thirty-six-ycet-old tcscutch chemist was referred ' [or analytic treatment because of complaints of severe chronic fatigue. excruciating. neatly con- stant headaches. weekncss. sexual malmlinstment. and various minor somatic complaints. 83: chance. he was on the toll and thin side. with a typical osthem’c [nonothlcu’c] build and impdllcct posture. l-lt's fatigability was such that the slightest exen tion was completely exhausting. He had gradually reduced his working time to a total of only four hours per day. When lfirst saw him. he was on the point 0! giving it up entirely. During thc remainder of his time. which was spent Strictly at home. he _ carefully alternated each fiftccn to thirty minutes of activity with one half-hour of reclining. Thcsc measures were torelly incficcrtvc in reduc- ing his fatigue. Hc had experienced progressive difficulty in concentration. Together with his sc- verc headaches. thcsc symptoms had resulted in an increasingly restricted social lit'c. a situation futct'gn to his prcvious nature. Life had bccomc so tntulctu- blc that he had bccn on the point of sutcidc He chose psychiatric treatment in what amounted to a lost desperate resort. bat-ing visitcd half a dozen physicians in various fields in his search for an organic basis and cure of his terrible symptoms. .. "air patient. however. proved - an apt candidate [or intensive treatment and even- tuolly. during the course of more than {our years of treatment—on a tbtce-tt'mcs. and later two-times a week b..st's—hc stepped having his headaches en- tirely. His fatigue also complctcly subsided. For more than two years he has worked full-rim: and has been able to resume his t'urmct level of social acriwty. (p. 486) 180 3’ ‘ Case 7 —-L. C—. a married woman. art 33, was admitted to hospitai on llctc‘ml-ct at. tozl. There was nothing noteworthy tn the family history save that her grand- father had been insane She had done well at school: was sulweqoently a telephone girl. until her marriage at the age of 26 She had been engaged for two years before. She had never become pregnant. Coitns occurred twice a week. With no contrateptise measures; there was dyspareunia soon after marriage and no gratification until recently. lier periods had always been nit-polar and she had been depressed just before the onset. Ten years hefOte (totS) she had a' ‘nen ous brealtdow’n .during which she was depressed and thought she was going mad. Since then she had been well and fond of amusement. though shy and somewhat reserved. . Her present illness had begun three months before w itb pains at the buds of her head and increasing Iistlessness. She gase up her usual aetis ities and did not eat properly. She felt sen frightened of rats. and thought she was not looleiug after the cat properly. She tried to take poison. She believed she was wicked and to blame for worrying others; she w as' ‘plotting against her husband s life and would be hanged for it ” She thought that her husband would die of consumption. She attempted to diown herself on the day of admission. . On admission she was restless and agitated. insisting that it was all her l. unit and that she must " go somewhere" She scratched her gums until they hled. saying. "1 thinlt 1 must do it" She asked where she had to no to. She said she was worried because of the dis-grace on the family through her. She said she was utterly tttiseralile and that sh: was. the cause of the trouble With the other patients. llcr skin was salluw and greasy. then-Owns an attnifotrtt rash on her face and back, her thyroid gland was slightly enlarged. There was fine. rapid tremor of both hands. her tongue was tremolons and she had alveolar pyorrluea. She continued very restless. she made slight attempts to commit suicide and kept asking what she should do, saying it was " all her fault 9'. By April. tom. she was less agitated. though she always looked bewildered and kept asking, " What shall we do for thg but 3 " She would wander about wringing her hands ; she said she ought not to take her food. Her physical and mental condition improved. and by the time she left hospital. August 29. 1929. she seemed quite well. Sol-se- quently she had very brief spells lasting perhaps a day. of mild depression and. now and then. she would get a ltttle too excited over some happening she w as looking forward to. For the most part. however. she has been ll\ ely and cheerful. In 193! she adopted the orphaned baby of a relative and has hrought it up quite well. She has somewhat altered her way of life since the illness. e. g. .. she always rests completely now for half an hour after lunch. During the six years since her illness she has not had any depression of the kind she remembers in her illness. 181 Va The patient was a 23-year-old engineer, who gave the following spontaneous description of his problem: “I am feeling very depressed. I feel as though I'm drag- ging myself down as well as my family. I have caused my parents no end of aggravation. The best thing would be if I dug a hole and buried myself in it. If I would get rid of myself, everybody would be upset for a time but then they would get over it. They would be better 06' without me.” The immediate life situation related to his depression was a job he had talten three months before. After graduating from college, he had had a succession of jobs and had started a small business that failed. He was not doing well in his current position and was certain that he would be fired within a few days. He experienced a gradual loss of self-confidence as his work did not seem to measure up to the ex- peetations of his employer. Two days before his psychiatric consultation he received notice that he would be fired. He became very discouraged and experienced a com— pleu loss of appetite and considerable difliculty in sleeping. He thought of various ways of hillinghimself, such as taking an overdose of pills or throwing himself from a high building. . A day before his consultation with me, he called his older brother to inform him that he was leaving town. His intention as that time was to commit suicide in a distant city. His brother suspected that something was wrong. so he came over to visit and to talk to him. After discussing the problem with his brother, the patient began to feel better. His brother told him he would lend him money to tide him over until he could get another position and he also made arrangements for the patient to start psychotherapy. The patient went to a football game that afternoon and began to feel better since his favorite team won. When I saw him the following day, he looked dejected and moderately de- pressed. He did not show any retardation or agitation. I administered the Depression Inventory (see Chapter 12). His cumulative score of 20 indicated a moderate de- pression. He acknowledged having the following symptoms: continual unremitting sadness; discouragement; feelings of being a failure; lack of satisfaction; guilt feel- ings for ”having let everybody down”; self-dislike; self-reproach; suicidal wishes; some loss of interest in other people; indecisiveness; insomnia; anorexia; and easy fatigability. 5. 182 Cas: )7 —E. \V. D—, a tramway EmlFfl- a-t 51, “'3‘ first admitted to hospital _ in May. 1927. His father had been a drunkard and was a patient in a hospital for criminal lunatics. where he died. A paternal uncle who died in a mental hospital and an aunt were also drunkards The patient worked as electrician until the war and snlN-qucntly as cable tori-man. He had married and his wife had one child when he was 3:. He had always been a serious man. somewhat grumpy; he was ia'msicmally intempcrate as to alcohol. but not often. After the strike in row he was reduced in rank and also in wages. In September. man. he was operated on for hernia. " His present illness had begun just before his operation for hernia- and had become gradually more evident. He had not been able to sleep and had felt low- spirited. He said that his inside was not \\ orking properly. (hi admission he was depressed. rather discontented. though without precise grounds. and complained that his bowels were not working because of the operation lor hernia. He said that some of his clothes were being worn by some of the other patients. ' He complained sometimes of trivial slights which he thought had been put upon , him. and said that he had been put in the worst bed in the ward. J-le improved ' steadily, became cheerful and amiable. and was discharged in july. 1017. He was admitted to the hospital again on June 3. r920. He had returned to his work as aganger; and continued at it until May. 1929. At the end of March. however, he had felt depressed and tired. and worried about a recurrence of his hernia. He had been promised an operation at St. Thomas's Hospital. where he was to enter as an in-patient on May 3o. As this time approached he became more depressed. and found that he could not put his mind to anything: he could not account for this and did not regard it as his own fault. He became prumm' and disagreeable at times and wandered of! to Southend one night and came home next morning. He had been getting on fairly well with his wife. who was. however. twenty years older than himself. There had been no coitus since his operation in '936- He had lost all sexual desire for the last three years. and had previously been afraid to attempt coitus for fear of causing a recurrence of his hernia. On admission he looked depressed, with wrinkled forehead and serious face: he looked also slightly discontented. His answers were terse and to the point. He said that the others disturbed him. that he felt low-spirited. and he had been worrying a great deal about his job. He did not think that he was ill except in 0" at)?“ “Mt: he was run down. and had congestion at the bottom of his atom . He became a little more cheerful. but was disinclined to net up because of rheumatism. which he said he had. He struck another patient and ra'casirrnally swore without provocation. He worried because he thought that his temperature wu. raised. At times he was restless and agitated. but at other times quiet and diner-fol. He improved further and admitted that he had been unduly irritable. .—--—— -—- ._' .m On LAugust as .henwas. translgred to another hospital for operation on his . . C . . He reappeared at the out-patient department on January 7. 193o. He had resumed work. but had continued depressed and irritable after the operation and had thrown up his job a week earlier. He was unwilling to return to hospital. and as he had been swearing at his wife. and threatening her. with intervals of showing excessive aflection. he was taken to the local observation ward. He had been talking constantly about his having lost his job; he became argumentative over tritles and blamed the world in general for his failure. He had ceased to worry about his hernia. He was admitted to a mental hospital in May. 1930. saying that his bones were drying up and were hollow. He said that everything round him was lousy and dirty. He said he would be better dead. out of the way: he muttered to himself and was occasionally blasphemous. He had threatened to drown himself and had attacked his wife and his daughter. On admission to the mental hospital there was no intellectual impairment. He said that he was now fit for nothing. He continued to believe that his bowels were stopped up and his bones hollow. But by May he said he thought his hollow bones were filling out. and two months later said he thought these ideas had been imaginary. He improved further and did clerical work in the hospital. In jannary. r931. his depression and hypochondriacal ideas returned in a milder form. He muttered to himself. ground his teeth and was disinclined to do work of any kind. The following year he worked in the stores and was free from gross delusions. though he worried about his physical health. deplorin'g his loss of energy. He had nothing to do with the other patients, and never wrote to his wife or daughter. He is now (April. 1936) doing work quite well in the stores. is on good terms with the other patients. laying billiards and chess with them. and seems content to stay in hospital. He as pulmonary emphysema and some arterio~sclcrosis. ‘ 183 LC“" 3-“5- M. B—. l student. 81. 22. female, was admitted to hospital on December 17. 1928. because she had been sent down from Oxford. had been sleeping badly and felt rather miserable. . - ° . Her father was a reserved. secltrsive, sensitive man. drank heavily 3‘ intervals; her mother " was jumpy ". excitable and irritable. Her youngest sister was rather excitable and had rare spells of depression. Her parents qu‘arrelled; she sidul ' with her father. and did not get on well with her mother. She had frequent attacks of rheumatism up to the age of to. night terrors and tantrums. She had bitten her nails. and for the last three years had stammered. She did very well at school. went to Oxford with a scholarship and : in the previous term sat for her final examination. She had not done well in this. -. Her periods had been regular ; latterly she had had a good deal of pain during her period, and felt irritable and depressed. Her sexual life had been devoid of incident; she said she had never felt sexual desire. nor fallen in love. She had been devoted to two girl friends. most unhappy when parted from them. She has always been reserved. umderately cheerful. quiet and smuewhat st'tlrrsive, though she got on well with a small circle at Oxford. She experienced tlu- kit-nest pleasure. almost ecstasy. when listening to oratnrirrs. She had indulged in fantasies of having children or teaching many girls. \‘ery sell-conscious, she dreaded enter- ing crowded tram-cars and felt as if she could burst out crying. She had always been " serious never went to dances or theatres. but sat at home and read modern ‘ poetry or blue-books and works on constitutional history. Her present illness had begun in the middle of my. after she had la-erfwnrking . hard. although she had been clreerfnl for a considerable period before this. She had begun to feel " fed-up " and Very miserable. She found she Could not get on with her work. She became gradually more miserable. was disinclined to associate with people and did pumly in her examination. On admission she was very quiet and showed scarcely any ernntinn save that she gnawed her knuckles and picked at her fingers. She jumped at any slight noise. She became “my pale when s'mm' lilurid was taken lrrurr her \eirr She tallied clearly and to the point. She said she was irulilrr-rent and saw ru- prrrpuse in life; she saw things now as they really were and not as illusions. She had lost all feelings for her friends. She thought she should have made an effort and denied that she was ill. She was rather obese and pale. She had two anxiety attacks soon after admission. on seeing another patient become upset. She continued to be miserable. and unable to see how maple could find any pleasure inliving. She said at times that for a brief interval she was feeling happy and energetic. Mostly she did not feel inclined to eat anything. During the rest of her stay in hospital she remained in the same condition. though occa- ' sionally she seemed more hopeful. She left hospital on June 29. 1929. still resigned and miserable. In September. 1929. she took a job in a private school. at which she has done well. She has remained reserved and quiet. but has become ruore cheerful. except when she is at home. In the judgment of her family she has been quite well since taking up work again. She is reticent. intolerant of noise. and " snappy " at the time of her menstrual periods. All her friends are women. In 1933 she returned to the University for a year and took her teaching diploma easily. She professes to be contented and well. a.” ----.- - . .u- I u o - 184 AJHFEFHJIXIIE MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Depart-ent of Psychology DEPARTMENTAL RESEARCH CONSENT PORN I have freely consented to take part in a scientific study being conducted by: Paula McNitt under the supervision of: Dozier Thornton Acadelic Title: Professor of Psycholo‘yfi_ The study has been explained to Ie and I understand the explanation that has been given and what Iy participation will involve. I understand that I an free to discontinue ay participation in the study 1 any tile without penalty. I understand that the results of the study will be treated in strict con- fidence and that I will relain anonylous. Nithin these restrictions. results of the study will be aade available to re at ay request. I understand that ay participation in the study does not guarantee any beneficial results to ae. I understand that, at my request, I can receive additional explanation of the study after my participation is coapleted. Signed: Date: 185 APPENDIX F mm: m: A STUDY Immoinnc m ”It“. WTIQBBIPS W PM mum MISS!“ in am WM: isto findoutnorsebouthowyouseeyourself endyourspouseinyournarriage. This studygeddtoourunderstanding of whether ad how the narriage relationship relates to a person's notional diffi- culties. ‘l'his my ultinstely assist in coinseling persons who are depressed and! or anxious. before we begin we need to review what the study involves. and what your rights are as a participant. First, you will be asked to attend an interviu with us. I'll be asking you about the probl- that led you to request help here at the VA lhntal health Clinic I'n not here to do therapy; that's your counselor's job. this interview will last fron one-half to one hour. at nest. You'll also be asked to coqlete several queatiomaires. we questionnaire asks for sons sinle background intonation about yourself. your job. your schooling. etc. the others ask about your relation- ship with your hsbandlwife during the past three nonths. and about how you are feeling at prunt. mess questions are straightforward: there's nothing ”sneaky” aboutthn. nerearenorigbtorwrongenswers. Inallthereareabout300 questions of thenltiple choice type. You'll need about o’ne and a half to two hours to anslssr thu. r... and your spouse will be paid I total of $5.00 for your participation. Thisisnys.llwayofseying"!hank-you.” Inaddition. ifyourequest, Iwill begladtoschsaleafeeoacksessionwithyouinwhichwecanst-ariaeanddis- cuss your answers. this 21 help you and your counselor to better understand your probl-e. Ihiscsnbeheldinabouttwoweeks. Severalnonths fronnowlcan provide a am of the findings of the entire study. There will be no poalty if you do not wish to participate in this study. You nay withdraw at ny tine without penalty. Your veteran's benefits will not be affected by your decision about participation. Please understand that all your answers will be kept strictly confidential. No one will be told about these unless you give as pernission, in writing, to infer: a particular person. also the study is reported you will not be identified by nans If you have any questions about the study or your participation, please ask, and I will answer th- for you. Signature: SUBJETANDMTB smmfi 186 APPENDIX G Guidelines for Interviewing the Clients 1. How are you today? Note: mood, other somatic complaints. 2. How long have you been in conseling with ? How frequently have you met with him/her? 3. Why did you come to the Center? What problems brought you here? Note: Depressive symptoms Anxiety Symptoms Family Problems Ask about depressive symptoms not mentioned:e4p, sleep difficulties, appetite or weight change. Ask about alcohol intake (especially if client is male). Assess: Which problem seems most serious/important? 4. How long have you had these problems? Note onset of depressive symptoms. Note onset of other symptoms or family problems. 5. Have you experienced problems before? If so, when? Note number and dates of previous depressive episodes. Note number and dates of other problem/symptom periods. 187 APPENDIX H 9.17 of Diagnostic Data To the Clinician: Please fill out this su-ary within two working days of your assess-ent interview. SyQtoI itens should be checked off (underline) only if these are observed or reported during the interview. Peel free to question the client about any synpton or problen he or she does not spontaneously report--if you suspect he has experienced it since his problen period began. For itens referring to history or nedication--by all mans check the client's (available) clinical records-4f this will help . 188 Client Clinician N"°= Name: Age: Place of Employment: Diagnosis (DSH II): . - Date: I. Depressive Synptons--Current A. Dysphoric Mood depressed, sad, blue, hopeless, discouraged, irritable, fearful, worried. tries a lot Physical Symptoms poor appetite, unintentional weight loss or gain (2 lbs. week) insomnia, hypersomnia, early morning wakening energy loss. fatigability agitation, retardation (speech, body movements) Other Psychological Symptoms loss of interest in usual activities, decreased sexual desire guilt, self reproach diminished ability to think or concentrate suicidal thoughts, wish to be dead, suicide attempt Evidence of Prior Manic Episodes client reports, case history evidence, Lithium treatment II. Other Past or Present Psychiatric or Medical Disorders A. Neurotic-Anxiety Disorders 1. Anxiety Neurosis--free floating anxiety with recurrent anxiety attacks present Yes No antedate depressive symptoms Yes No predominate over depressive symptoms Yes No 2. Phobic Neurosis--obvious phobias present Yes No antedate depressive symptoms Yes No predominate over depressive symptoms Yes No 3. Hysteria (conversion symptoms, fugue states. chronic, multiple, vague unexplained medical problems)~ present . Yes No antedate depressive symptoms Yes No predominate over depressive symptoms Yes No III. IV. 189 4. Obsessive-Compulsive Neurosis-~obsessional thoughts. impulses, com- pulsive behaviors present Yes NO antedate depressive symptoms Yes No predominate over depressive symptoms Yes No D. Alcoholism-Alcohol Abuse (does not include heavy drinking consequent to depressive symptoms) present Yes No antedate depressive symptoms Yes No predominate over depressive symptoms Yes N° C. Drug Dependency (include gl1_street or prescription drugs) current Yes No past Yes No D. Antisocial Personality (Check only if strong evidence of chronic delin- quency, antisocial behavior, vagrancy, m vocational and/or marital mdladjustment). Yes No E. Schizophrenia current (evidence of overt psychotic symptoms) Yes No in remission (evidence--history or medication) Yes NO P . Sexual "Deviations" homosexuality pederasty trans—sexuality voyeurism 6. Organic Drain Syndrome H. Mental Retardation I. Serious or Life-Threatening Illness cancer kidney heart disease other (specify) History of Depressive Symptoms A. Length of Present Episode less than 3 weeks 6 weeks to 6 months 3-6 weeks 6 months or longer 3. Previous Episodes? 0, l, 2, 3, 4, S, 6, or more than 6 C. Most Recent Previous Episode? within past 6 months within 5 years within year more than five years ago Treatment History A. Hospitalization hospitalized for present condition How long? past psychiatric hospitalizations O, l, 2, 3. 4, S, 6, more than 6 190 B. Outpatient treatment Psychotherapy or psychiatric consulthtion (at least 2 consults) 1. This or other mental health center 0, l, 2. 3, 4. 5, 6, more than 6 2. Private mental health pfactitioner O, l, 2, 3, 4, S, 6, more than 6 3. Family physician (e.g., for valium prescription) 0.1, 2, 3, 4. 5, 6, more than 6 C. Kinds of Treatment Received individual therapy family BTWP therapy medical/somatic marital D. Medicationucurrently prescribed (by trade name) 1. Anti-depressant medication Tofranil, Norpramin. Elavil, Aventyl, Nardil, Harplan, Parnate, Lithium carbonate, other 2. Minor tranquilizers Valim, Librim, Miltown, Tranxene 3. Anti-psychotic medication Thorazine, Stelazine, Haldol. Mellaril, Nevane, Prolixin Su-ary Is this client depressed? Yes No Mildly Moderately Severely Is depression the cl ient's primary problem? Yes No If no, depression appears secondary to Is this the client's first depressive episode? Yes No first psychiatric disorder of any kind? Yes No 191 APPENDIX I General Information Sheet ID 0: Age: Education (highest level completed): Occupation: Working now? Rel ig ion: Protestant Cathol ic Jewish Other None Race: Caucasian Negro Oriental About your marriage: How long have you and your spouse been married? During the past three months have you and your wife/husband been separated for more than a week's time? If “yes", how many times? This marriage is your: first second third fourth fifth? How many children do you have? How many' children are living in your home? __;I 192 APPENDIX J Beck's Depression Inventory I.D. I N.H.C. Age: Sex: IBCK'S 0.! Instructions: Please read each set of statements completely, then circle the one which most represents how you—feel right m. For example, read all the statsnsnts ill category "A". reflect for a minute, then choose one of also and circle it. Then continue to the next set until you have chosen one statement for every letter through "0". A. I do not feel sad. I feel blue or sad. I am blue or sad all the time-and I can't snap out of it. I am so sad or unhappy that it is quite painful. I am so sad orunhappy that I can't stand it. I. I am not particularly pessimistic or discouraged about the future. I feel discouraged about the future. I feel I have nothing to look forward to. I feel that I won't ever get over my troubles. I feel that the future is hopeless and tlmt things canmt improve. C. I do not feel like a failure. I feel I have failed are than the average person. I feel I have accomplished very little that is worthwhile or that means anything. ‘ As I look back on my life all I can see is a lot of failures. I feel I am a complete failure as a person (parent, husband, wife)- D. I am not particularly dissatisfied. I feel bored most of the time. I don't enjoy things the way I used to.‘ I don't get satisfaction out of anything any more. I am dissatisfied with everything. a. I don't feel particularly guilty. I feel badorulnlorthyagoodpart ofthe time. I feel quite guilty. I feel bad or unnrthy practically all the time now. I feel as dough I am very bad or worthless. P. I don't feel I am being punished. I have a feeling that something bad may happen to me. I feel I am being mished or will be punished. I feel I deserve to be punished. I want to be punished. 6. don't feel disappointed in myself. I I am disappointed in myself. I don't like myself. ‘ I am disgusted with myself. I hate myself. 193 I ubn't feel I as any worse than anybody else. I am critical of myself for my weaknesses or mistakes. I blame myself for my faults. I blame myself for everything bad that happens. I don't have any thoughts of harming myself. I have thoughts of harming myself. but I would not carry them out. I feel I would be better off dead. ' I feel my fully would be better off if I were dead. have definite plans about co-itting suicide. d kill myself if I could. t cryan'y more than usuml. 33” 5 g E E 3 can't stop it. able to cry but now I can't cry at all even though I want a f i" .5 I I I I I I :ai 8 8' no more irritated now than I ever am. . annoyed or irritated are easily than I used to. irritated all the time. 't get irritated at all at the things that used to irritate me. :33 not lost interest in other people. ass interested in other people now than I used to be. lost most of my interest in other people and have little feeling - tho. pas V ail? s-e Hess-s ll. lost all my interest in other people and don't care about them at have al I make decisions about as well as ever. I try to put off nking decisions. I have great difficulty in nking decisions. I can't aka any decisions at all any are. I don't feel I look any worse than I used to. I am worried that I am looking old or unattractive. I feel that there are pernnent changes in my appearance and thOY ”119 '9 look unattractive. I feel that I u ugly or repulsive looking. Icanworkaboutaswell asbefora. It takes extra effort to get started at doing something. I don't work as well as I used to. I laws to push myself very hard to do anything. I can't lb any work at all. I can sleep as wall as usual. I wake up more tired in the morning than I used to. I wake up l-Z houurs earlier than usual and find it hard to get back to sleep. I wake up early every day and can't‘ get more than 5 hours sleep. U. 194 I don't get any are tired than usuml. I get tired more easily than I used to. I get tired from doing anything. . I get too tired to do anything. “ My appetite is no worse than usual . My appetite is not as good as it used to be. My appetite is much worse now. I have no appetite at all any more. haven't lost much weight, if any, lately. have lost Dre than 5 pounds. have lost more than 10 pounds. have lost are tlmn 15 pounds. NHH.-O am no are concerned about my health than usual. am concerned about aches and pains or upset Sumch or constipation. am so concerned with how I feel or what I feel tlult it's hard to think of Itch else. I am coqletely absorbed in what I feel. HHH have not noticed any recent change in my interest in sex. am less interested in sex than I used to be. am much less interested in sex now. have lost interest in sex completely. HHHH 195 APPENDIX K FORM F A MARITAL COMMUNICATION INVENTORY DEVELOPED BY MILLARD .l. BlliNVl-INl'. SR. 1979 Revision This inventory offers you an opportunity to make an ohjcctiyc study ol‘thc Liquor and patterns of communication in your marital relationship. It it ill cntuhlc y on and your husband to better understand each other. We hclicuc you uill liuutl it both interesting and helpful to make this study. DIRECTIONS I. Please answer each question as quickly as you can according to the way you lccl a! the moment (not the way you usually feel or felt lust neck.) 2. Do not consult your husband while completing this inycntory. You may discuss it with him after both of you hate completed it. Remember that the counseling value of this form will be lost if you change um- utusucr during or after this discussion. 3. Honest answers are l'l'l'l' necessary if this form is to be of \aluc. Please be as frank as possible. Your answers are confidential. Your name is not rcquircd. 4. Use the following examples for practice. Put a check ( ) in Ullt' of the four blanks on the right to show how the question applies to your lulurriuugc. Ml“?- Isl \ll\ I|\lt.\ HI'HNHI \Iflill Does your husband talk about his real feelings? Does he let you know when his feelings are hurt‘.’ 5. Read each question carefully. If you cannot give the exact answer to a qucsl iouu. answer the best you can but be sure to answer each one. There are no right or wrong answers. Answer according to the way you fch m (In: prt-swu (film. I upyrlght I"! “land 1. Ilrmenu. M. W rights resent-d. Printed in the l nilrd Slates of uni-tics. Published by l “Ill \ II” Pt II It \mns. l\t Rm 421. haluda. \.t . 28773 196 . Do you and your husband discuss the manner in which the family income should be spent? . Does he discuss his work and interests with you? Do you have a tendency to keep your feelings to yourself? Is your husband‘s tone of voice irritating? Does he have a tendency to say things which would be better left unsaid? Are your mealtime conversations easy and pleasant? Do you find yourself keeping after him about him faults? Does he seem to understand your feelings? Does your husband nag you? Does he listen to what you have to say? . Does it upset you to a great extent when your husband is angry with you? Does he pay you compliments and say nice things to you? . Is it hard to understand your husband's feelings and attitudes? Is he affectionate toward you? . Does he let you finish talking before responding to what you are saying? Do you and your husband remain silent for long periods when you are angry with one another? . Does he allow you to pursue your own interests and activities even if they are different from his? . Does he try to lift your spirits when you are ‘depressed or discouraged? . Do you avoid expressing disagreement with him because you are afraid he will get angry? . Does your husband complain that you don‘t under- stand him? . Do you let your husband know when you are displeased with him? . Do you feel he says one thing but really means another? . Do you help him understand you by saying how you think. feel. and believe? . Are you and your husband able to disagree with one another without losing your tempers? .80“!- lhl \l.l.\ TIMES SHIN!“ 8}} l." 197 SOME. l'SlTAIJJ TIMES SELDOM VIEWER 25. Do the two of you argue a lot over money? 26. When a problem arises between you and your husband are you able to discuss it without losing control of your emotions? 27. Do you find it difficult to express your true feelings to him? 7 28. Does he offer you cooperation. encouragement a and emotional support in your role (duties) as a wife? 29. Does your husband insult you when angry with you? 30. Do you and your husband engage in outside in- terests and activities together? 3|. Does your husband accuse you of not listening to what he says? _ 32. Does he let you know that you are important to him? 33. Is it easier to confide in a friend rather than your husband? 34. Does he confide in others rather than in you? 35. Do you feel that in most matters your husband knows what you are trying to say? 36. Does he monopolize the conversation very much? __ 37. Do you and your husband talk about things which are of interest to both of you? _ 38. Does your husband sulk or pout very much? _ __ __ __ 39. Do you discuss sexual matters with him? __ __ ____... __ 40. Do you and your husband discuss your personal problems with each other? 4|. Can your husband tell what kind of day you have had without asking? __ __ __ __ 42. Do you admit that you are wrong when you know that you are wrong about something? ._ __ __ _ 43. Do you and your husband talk over pleasant things that happen during the day? __ __ _ .— 44. Do you hesitate to discuss certain things with your husband because you are afraid he might hurt ' your feelings? __ __ __ __ 45. Do you pretend you are listening to him when actually you are not really listening? _ _ __ __ 46. Do the two of you ever sit down just to talk things over? .__.. __ __ __ 198 ABOUT YOU Read the following sentences and complete them with the first thing that comes to your mind. It is important for you and your spouse to agree that you will not hold anything against each other for expressing your views. Your goal is to better understand each other. so please be frank in order to benefit as much as you can Irom this activity. I. LATELY. OUR RELATIONSHIP Es) THE MAIN PROBLEM I SEE FACING US AT THIS TIME IS— 3. ABOUT MY SPOUSE. l APPRECIATE: a. b. 4. TWO THINGS I WANT FROM MY SPOUSE THAT I'M NOT GETTING: a. b. 5. IT WOULD HELP OUR RELATIONSHIP IF I 6. I'M WILLING TO General Informatlion: Your Age;— Husband's Age____ Length of Present Marriage _ Your Religious Preference.___ Your Husband‘s Preference Have You Ever Been Married. Divorced. or Widowed Before? YES NO If YES. Please Explain Your Education Occupation H usband's Education H is Occupation Your C hildren's Ages: Ages of Boys Ages of Girls 199 APPENDIX L S-10-68 (copyrighted) Scale of Feelings and Behavior of Love Clifford H. Swensen and Frank Gilnsr Purdue University, St. Louis University (Modified by Paula C. McNitt. 1980) iThis scale contains itens describing the nany ways in which people feel . they express love. Some of these iteas describe things said between people who love each other, some describe feelings people have fer people who- they love, and some describe things people do fbr people they love. ‘ When you answer the itens for your relationship with your wife be sure to mark an answer fer every iten. Each iten has three choices. Mark the choice that cones closest to describing the way you behave, talk or feel toward your wife as your relationship exists a: thg_present tine. This leans during the past three months and gg£_just today. There is no time linit, but you should nark your answer to the items as rapidly as you can. 200 Ybur wife tells you that she feels you get along well together. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She freggently tells you this. Your wife tells you that she wants to live up to your expectations fer her. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally.tells you this. e) She fregggntly tells you this. Year wife tells you that she feels a good "spirit? in the things she does with you. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She fregggntly tells you this. Ybur wife tells you that she feels free to talk about anything with you. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. e) She freguently tells you this. Your wife tells you that she trusts you completely. a) She never tells you this. b) She oecasionall tells you this. e) She freggently tells you this. You tell your wife that you feel safe when you are with her. a) Ybu never tell her this. b) YOu occasionally tell her this. c) Ybu fEESEEntly tell her this. You tell your wife that you feel that your relationship has improved with tine. a) You never tell her this. b) Ybu occasionally tell her this. c) Ybu frequently tell her this. Ybur wife tells you that the thought of you dying disturbs her. a)- She never tells you this. b) She OEEigionally tells you this. c) She freggently tells you this. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 201 Your wife tells you that she feels your relationship has leproved with tile. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. e) She frequently tells you this. Yeu tell your wife that you feel that you understand each other. a) Yea never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. e) You frequently tell her this. You tell your wife that you don't have to put up a 'false front" around her. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fregggntly tell her this. Yeu tell your wife that you have a warn, happy feelirg when you are with her. a) You never tell her this. b) Ybu occasionall tell her this. c) You fregggntly tell her this. You tell your wife that you have faith in her. a) You never tell her this. b) Ybu occasionally tell her this. c) You freguently tell her this. Ybu tell your wife that you want to live up to her expectations fer you. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionall tell her this. c) You fregggntly tell her this. You tell your wife that you feel lore cheerful, optimistic and confident when you are with her. a) Ybu never tell her this. b) Ybu occasionall tell her this. c) Ybu fn ggently tell her this. You tell your wife that you feel a "good spirit" in the things you do with her. a) You never tell her this. b) Yeu occasionally tell her this. c) Ybu fregggntly tell her this. 17. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 202 You tell your wife that you trust her coupletely. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fregggntly tell her this. Yeu tell your wife that you feel she is important and worthwhile. a) You never tell her this. b) Ybu occasionall tell her this. c) You fregggntly tell her this. . Yeu tell your wife that you feel free to talk about anything with her. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionall° tell her this. c) Ybu freguentiy toll her this. You tell your wife that you feel her expectations of you are not too great--they are reasonable. a) Ybu never tell her this. b) Ybu oceasionallv tell her this. c) You fTEQuentlv tell her this. Yeur wife tells you whether or not she does anything special to aaintain or ilprove her appearance, such as, diet, exercise, etc. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She fregggntly tells you this. Ybur wife tells you the kind of behavior in others which annOys her, or lakes her furious. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She freguentli’tells you this. Ybur wife tells you her thoughts and feelings about religious groups other than her own. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She freggently tells you this. Ybur wife tells you whether or not she plans sole major decision in the near future--such as, returning to school, getting a new job, having a baby, buying soaething big. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She f¥Egggntly_tells you this. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 203 Ybur wife tells you her favorite jokes-~the kind of jokes she likes to hear. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She ffigggntly tells you this. You tell your wife what particularly anmys you at st about your closest friend. - a) You have never told her this. b) You have occasionall told her this. c) You have ffzgggntly told her this. You tell your wife things about your own personality that worry or anno you. ' ' . a) You have never told her this. b) You have occasionall told her this. c) Ybu have ffzgggntly told her this. Ybur wife tells you what her chief health concern, worry, or problel is at the present tine. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She fEEQEgntIy tells you this. Ybur wife tells you about her sparetile hobbies or interests. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She fregggntly tells you this. . Your wife tells you what particularly annoys her nest about her closest friend. ' . a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She f5eguently tells you this. You tell your wife what the chief pressures and strains in your daily work are. a) Ybu never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fregggntly tell her this. Ybu tell your wife things about the future that worry you at present. a) Ybu never tell her this. b) Ybu occasionall tell her this. c) You Wad tell her this. 33. 35. 36. 37. 39. 40. 204 You tell your wife what you are most sensitive about. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fregggntlz tell her this. You tell your wife the kind of behavior in others that most anncys you. or lakes you furious. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fregggntlz tell.her this. You tell your wife what you regard as your chiif handicap to Joins a better job in your work or school. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You freggentlz tell her this. You tell your wife what your strongest albitiot.iu at the present time. a) Ybu never tell her this. . b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You freggentli tell her this. You tell your wife whether or not you plan some eajor decision in the near future--getting a new job, starting a new busine ss, moving to a new home, buying something big. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fregggntliitell her this. Yeur wife tells you the chief pressures and strains in ner daily work. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She freguentli tells you this. Your wife tells you what she is most sensitive abcut. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She f?eguentIz_tells you this. Your wife tells you her views about what is acceptable sex morality for people to fellow. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She freguentlz tells you this. 41. 42. 45. You is 1 46. ‘ 47, 48. 41. 42. 43. 45. 46. 47. 48. 205 Your wife tells you the things about her appearance that she likes IOSt ‘r is proudest of. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She fregggntly tells you this. You provide support for your wife's food, clothing and housing. a) Yeu never do this. b) You occasionall do this. c) You freguently Eb this. You sacrifice your own needs. such as clothes, in crder to provide for your wife. a) You never do this. b) YOu occasionally do this. c) You fregggntli—do this. Your wife gives you an accurate knowledge of her sex life up to the pre- sento-the names of sex partners in the past, if any; her ways of getting sennal gratification. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionall tells you this. c) She In magently tells you this. Yeur wife tells you what she feels the guiltiest about, or most ashamed of in her past. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally has mentioned such things. c) She alwa tells—you when she has done something she feels very guilty or asfiim i about. Your wife tells you the characteristics of her mother that she does not like or did not like. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She freggently tells you this. You tell your wife whether or not you have sex problems and the nature of these problems. a) You never tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You fggguently tell her this. You feel that you don't have to put up a "false front" around your wife. a) You neLer feel this way. b) You occasionally feel this way. c) You have fgggggntly felt this way. 49. SO. 51. $2. 53. SS. 56. 206 You give your wife an accurate knowledge of your sex life up to the pre- sent--the names of your sex partners in the past if any; your WPYS 0f getting sexual gratification. a) You neLer tell her this. b) You occasionally tell her this. c) You Ezeguentliitell her this. You tell your wife what you feel the guiltiest about, or ncst ashamed of in the past. a) You never told her this. b) You occasionally have aentioned such things. c) You fre ntl tell her when you have done something you feel guilty or :EEIEEE‘:E§bz. Your wife shows love by’a willingness to change or give up her ideals in order to please you. a) She never does this. b) She occasionall" does this. c) She frguently foes this. You provide money or support for her education, a new business venture. or other interest. a) You never do this. b) You occasionall do this. c) You fregugntly 55 this. Ybur wife is like another person that you have loved, such as a relative. a) You nLver feel this way. b) You occasional F—TL¥ feel this way. c) You ent eel this way. You do things or go places with your wife even though these activities don't particularly appeal to you. a) You never do this. b) You occasionall do this. c) Yen fregggntly 50 this. You show love for your wife by a willingness to change or give up ideals for her. a) Yen never do this. b) YOu.occasionally do this. c) You frequently do this. You teach your wife values and ideals in life. a) Ybu never do this. b) You SEEEEionally do this. c) You greguently do this. 57. $8. 59. 60e 61. 62. 63. 65. 207 You discipline your wife. a) You never do this. b) You occasionally do this. c) You freguently do this. The differences that come up between you do ggt_lizrupt the relationship. a) Our differences fre entl disrupt the relationship. b) Our differences occas anally disrupt the relaticnship. c) Our differences never disrupt the relationsh p. 3 “H You pray for your wife. a) You never do this. b) You occasionally do this. c) You freguently do this. Your wife tells you that she wants you to agree with her when she is in an argument with a third person. a) She never tells you this. b) She occasionally tells you this. c) She freguently tells you this. Your wife can trust you because you are honest and truthful with her. a) Your wife can never trust you. b) Your wife can occasionally trust you. c) Your wife can freguently trust you. Your wife teaches you values and ideals in life. a) She never teaches you values and ideals. b) She occasionally teaches you values and ideals. c) She freggently teaches you values and ideals. You listen with interest when your wife talks. a) Yen never do this. b) Yen occasionall do this. c) You freguentlz do this. Your wife teaches you skills, such as how to drive a car or how to sew, cook, fix things at home. a) She never teaches you skills. b) She occasionally teaches you skills. c) She fregggntly teaches you skills. Your wife can be trusted because she has been honest and truthful with you. a) You can never trust her. b) You can occasionall trust her. c) You can fregggntly trust her. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 20E! Your wife approves of you. a) She never does. b) She occasionallé does. c) She fregugntly s. You write or telephone your wife when you are separated. a) You never do. b) You occasionall do c) You fregggntly go. You approve of your wife. a) You never do. b) You occasionally do. c) You fregggntly‘do. You are 225 over demanding of your wife, but are causiderate of her time, energy, etc . ' a) You are mgge rtly over demanding. b) You are occasionally over demanding. c) You are never ever demanding. You encourage your wife when she is discouraged. a) You never do this. b) You occasionally do this. c) You freguently’do this. Your wife shows an interest in you and your work. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She fzggiently does. Your wife is ngg_over demanding of you, but is considerate of your oon time, energy, etc.‘ a) She fre entl is ever demanding. b) She occasionally is over demanding. c) She never is over demanding. Your wife gives you encouragement when you are discouraged. a) She never does. b) She occasionall does. c) She freguently goes. Your wife allows you to make the final decisions in the things that are of prieary iaportance to you. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She freguently does. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 209 Your wifo is respectful and considerate of your opinions. a) She never is. b) She occasionally is.‘ c) She grgggentli is. Your with is even-tempered and kind in dealings with you. a) She never is. b) She occasionall is. c) She fraggently Es. You try to live up to your wife's ideals and expectations for you. a) You never try to. b) You occasionall try to. c) You freggently try to. You are respectful and considerate of your wifo's opinions. a) You never are. b) You occasionall are. c) You frT—Tz' 2m zare. You are courteous to your wife. a) You never are. b) You occasionall are. c) You freggently are. You are even-tempered and'kind in your dealings with your wife. a) You never are. b) You occasionall are. c) You frgggentiy are. Your wife tries to get you in a good mood when you are angry. a) She never tries. b) She occasionally tries. c) She fregggntly tries. Your wife gives you good or useful advice. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She freguently does. Your wife disciplines you. a) She never does. b) She occasionall .does. c) She frggggntiy 55.3. 85. 86. 87. 89. 90. 91. 92. 210 You obey your wife. a) You never do. b) You occasionally do. c) You freggentlz do. Your wife prays for you. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She freggently does. You try to get your wife in a good need when she i: angr'. a) You never do this. b) You occasionall do this. c) You H guenty this. _ Your wife runs errands for you--to the store, etc. a) She never does this. b) She occasionally dues this. c) She fregggntly does this.. Your wife loans objects of value to you--such as a :1?- a) She never does this. b) She occasionall does this. c) She freguently goes this. Your wife provides constructive criticism when you neei it. a) She never does this. b) She occasionally does this. c) She frgguentlz does this. Your wife directly protects you froe harm or bocfly in;trY- a) She never does this. b) She occasionally does this. c) She freguently does this. If you support your wife, she tries to be economical in her expenditures. a) She never tries to be, or this item does not apply. b) She occasionally tries to be. c) She fregggntly tries to be. Your wife secures favors for you, such as getting a job, :pecial appoint- ments, discounts on things you want to buy. a) She never does this, or this item does not apply. b) She occasionally does this. c) She fregggntly does this. 93. 95. 97. 99. 100. 211 You run errands for your wife--to the store, etc. a) You never do this. b) You occasionally do this. c) You freguently do this. You secure favors for your wife, such as obtaining a job. special appoint- seats, or a discount on something she wants to hnv. a) You ngggg_do this, or this item does not aptl'. b) You occasionall do this. c) You freguently do this. Your wifo provides support for food, clothing ans housing for YOU- a) She never does. . b) She occasionall does. c) She reguent y oes. Your wife performs chores for you-~washing or ironiig clothes, typing. driving you around, etc. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She fregugntly does. Your wifo helps you with tasks such as homework, household tasks. garden work, etc. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She fregggntly‘does. Your wifo serVes on things you are involved in, such as spcnsor for a club to which you belong. a) She never does. b) She occasionally does. c) She freggentlifidoes. Your wife provides direct support when you are in difficulties, such as intervening when you are in conflict with school authorities, your bcss, or the police, etc. a) She never does. b) She occasionall does. c) She ffeggently goes. Your wife provides Ioney or support for your education, a new business venture, or some other interest. a) She never does this. b) She occasionally does this. c) She frigugntlz‘does this. 212 APPENDIX M SASB Questionnaires DIRECTIGIS for IN'I'REX QESTIGINAIRES This questionnaire asks you to rate ways you feel about you'self and sole sir nificant others. You are asked to rate each question on a scale of 0 (never, not at all) to me (always, perfectly). The analysis of the questionnaire organises your answers in ways which can help you thd why you feel the way you do. People who go ahead and answer honestly, avoiding any teeptatim to “whitewash" or "paint a rosy picture" usu- ally ara very pleased with the results. If you agree, the results can be made available to you to help you with your understanding of you-self. Please answer the questions for how you really think or feel. Your initial reaction to each question will most often be your best answer. If a question offends you, score it were or leave it blank. There are no ”right" or "wrong” answers. It ‘s you- view fluid: is important-«not what is necessarily "tune”, "false" or what someone else eight think you should say. Put your answers on the proper answer sheets making sure you match the letter of the questionnaire with the letter of the answer sheet. lien you've fin- ished, give the questionnaire and the answer sheet back to the person who gave you these materials. The results will be returned as quickly as possible. 213 INTREX FORM C’ Please use the answer sheet marked "C" and indicate how well each question describes your wife. Use the scale which appears at the top of the answer sheet. Constructively, sensibly, persuasively analyzes situations involving me. Has her own separate identity, internal standards. Insists I follow har norms and rules so that I do things 'pmpcrlyfl' . Puts me down, tells me I do things all wrong, tells me her ways are super- ior. Learns from as, takes advice from as. Just does things my way without much fooling of her own. is apathetic. ODIN" GUI 7. Angrily leaves me out, absolutely refuses to have anything to do with as. 3- "TIJY. comfortably accepts help, caregiving when I offer it. 9. Does her own thing'by doing the exact opposite of what I want. 10. Is straightforward. Clearly expresses her positions so I can give them due consideration. ll. Enthusiastically shows, shares herself or "thing" with me. 12. Tortures, aurders, annihilates me no aatter what I do because "I'a as." 13. Does strange, irrelevant, unrelated things with what I say or do; goes on her "own trip." 14. Ecstatically, joyfully, exuberantly, lovingly responds to me sexually. ls. Harlly, cheerfully invites me to be in touch with her as often as I want. 16. Warmly, happily keeps in contact with me. 17. Freely comes and goes as she pleases. 18. Out of great love for me, she tenderly, lovingly touches ae sexually if I seen receptive. l9. Stimulates and teaches me, shows me how to understand, do. 20. Accuses and blames as; tries to get me to admit I am wrong. 21. Enthusiastically, very lovingly shows me how glad she is to see as just as I am. 22. Looks to me, depends on no to take care of everything for her. 23. Harshly punishes me, takes revenge, makes me suffer greatly. 24. Understands as well, shows empathy and warmth even if I don't see things as she does. 1980, INTREX Interpersonal Institute, Inc. 214 25. Is trusting. Asks for what she wants and counts on me to be kind and con- siderate. 26. willingly accepts, yields to my reasonable suggestions, ideas. 27. Screams, agonizes, protests desperately that I am destroying. killinl hfir- 28. Gently strokes me verbally and/or physically; she lovingly gives me pleasure with "no strings attached." 29. Intrudes, blocks, restricts me. 30. Even though very suspicious and distrustful of me, she goes along with I? arguments, ideas. 31. Obeys my preforred rules, standards, routines. 32. Rips me off, gouges me, grabs all she can from me. 33. Pleasing me is so important that she checks with me on every little thing. 34. Is obviously terrified, very fearful of me; is extremely wary. 3S. Misleads, deceivas, deludes and diverts me. 36. In a very grouchy, surly manner, she goes along with my needs and wants. 37. Provides for, nurtures, takes care of’me. 38. Lets me speak freely and can be trusted to negotiate fairly even if we dis- agree. 39. Ignores me, just doesn't notice as at all. 40. Uncaringly lets me go, do what I want. 41. Snarls angrily, hatefully refuses my caregiving, my offers to assist. 42. Filled with rage and/or fear, she does what she can to escape, flee or hide from me. 43. Believing it's for my own good, she checks on me and reminds me of what I should do. 44. Gives me her "blessing" and leaves me free to develop my own separate iden- tity. 45. Forgets me, just doesn't remember our agreements, plans. 46. Gives in and does things the way I want, but sulks quietly with resentment and anger. 47. Yields, submits, gives in to me. 48. Approaches me very menacingly; hurts me very badly if she gets a chance. 49. Manages, controls me, takes charge of everything. 50. Leaves me to do things on my own because she believes I am competent. Sl. Expresses her thoughts in a clear and friendly manner so I have every opportunity to understand her well. 52. Feels, thinks, becomes what she thinks I want. 53. Leaves me to starve, to get what I vitally need all on my own. 54. Actively listens, accepts and affirms me as a person even if our views dis- agree. 215 Angrily detaches from me, doesn't ask for anything; weeps alone about me. Pays close attention in order to anticipate all my needs; takes care of absolutely everything for me. Ihines, protests, tries to explain, justify, account for herself. Asserts, holds her own without needing external support. Avoids me by being busy and alone with her "own thing." larmly shows how'much she likes and appreciates me just exactly as I an. 61. 62. 63. 65. 66. Halls herself off’from me, doesn't hear, doesn't react. Relaxes, enjoys, really lets go with me. Feels wonderful about being with me. Believing it's for my own good, she tells me exactly what to do, he, think. Buries her rage and resentment and scurries to appease me to avoid my dis- approval. Approaches me with unwarranted, even crazy ideas about me, and doesn’t notice how or if I respond. Goes her own separate way. 67. 69. 70. 71. 72. Looks after my interests, takes steps to protect me, actively backs me up. Freely and openly discloses her innermost self so I can truly know "who she is." Is joyful and exuberant and expects to have wonderful fun with me. Just when she is needed most, she abandons me, leaves me "in the lurch." Neglects me, my interests, needs. Leaves me free to do and be whatever I want. "h 216 For qu°8t10ns 073-144, change from rating her to rating yourself in this rela- tionship. Continue using the same scale at the top of answer sheet "C." 73. I constructively, sensibly, persuasively analyze situations involving her. 74. l have my own separate identity, internal standards. 75. I insist she follow my norms and rules so that she does things "prOPOTIY-" 76. I put her down, tell her she does things all wrong, tell her my HEY! are superior. 77. I learn from her, take advice from her. 78. I just do things her way without much feeling of my own: I am apathetic. 79. I angrily leave her out, absolutely refuse to have anything to do with her. 80. I warmly, comfortably accept help, caregiving when she offers it. 81. I do my own thing by doing the exact opposite of what she wants. 82. I am straightforward. I clearly express my positions so that she can give them due consideration. a 83. I enthusiastically show, share myself or "thing" with her. 84. I torture, murder, annihilate her no matter what she does just because she is who she is. 85. I do strange, irrelevant, unrelated things with what she says or does; I go on my "own trip." 86. I ecstatically, joyfully, exuberantly, lovingly respond to her sexually. 87. I warmly, cheerfully invite her to be in touch with me as often as she wants. 88. I warmly, happily keep in contact with her. 89. I freely come and go as I please. 90. Out of great love for her, I tenderly, lovingly touch her sexually if she seems receptive. 91. I stimulate and teach her, show her how to understand, do. 92. I accuse and blame her; try to get her to admit she is wrong. 93. I enthusiastically, very lovingly show her how glad I am to see her just as she is. 94. I look to her, depend on her to take care of everything for me. 95. I harshly punish her, take revenge, make her suffer greatly. 96. I understand her well, show empathy and warmth even if she doesn't see things as I do. 97. I am trusting. I ask for what I want and count on her to be kind and con- siderate. 98. I willingly accept, yield to her reasonable suggestions, ideas. 99. I scream, agonize, protest desperately that she is destroying, killing me. 100. I gently stroke her verbally and/or physically; I lovingly give her pleasure with "no strings attached." 101. I intrude, block, restrict her. 102. Even though very suspicious and distrustful of her, I go along with her arguments, ideas. 217 103. I obey her preferred rules, standards, routines. 104. I rip her off, gouge her, grab all I can from her. 105. Pleasing her is so important that I check with her on every little thing. 106. I am obviously terrified, very fearful of her; I am extremely wary. 107. I mislead, deceive, delude and divert her. 108. In a very grouchy, surly manner, I go along with her needs and wants. 109. I provide for, nurture, take care of her. 110. I let her speak freely and can be trusted to negotiate fairly 0V9" if '9 disagree. 111. I ignore her, just don't notice her at all. 112 I uncaringly let her go, do what she wants. 113. I snarl angrily, hatefully refuse her caregiving, her offers to assist. 114. Filled with rage and/or fear, I do what I can to escape. flee, or hid! from her. 115. Believing it's for her own good, I check on her and remind her of what she should do. ‘ 116. I give her my "blessing" and leave her free to develop her own separate identity. 117. I forget her, just don't remember our agreements, plans. 118. I give in and do things the way she wants, but sulk quietly with resent- ment and anger. 119. I yield, submit, give in to her. 120. I approach her very menacingly; I hurt her very badly if I get a chance. 121. I manage, control her, take charge of everything. 122. I leave her to do things on her own because I believe she is competent. 123. I express my thoughts in a clear and friendly manner so she has every opportunity to understand me well. 124. I feel, think, become what I think she wants. 125. I leave her to starve, to get what she vitally needs all on her own. 126. I actively listen, accept and.affirm her as a person even if our views disagree. 127. I angrily detach from her, don't ask for anything; I weep alone about her. 128. I pay close attention in order to anticipate all her needs; I take care of absolutely everything for her. 129. I whine, protest, try to explain, justify, account for myself. 130. I assert, hold my own without needing external support. 131. I avoid her by being busy and alone with my "own thing." 132. I warmly show how much I like and appreciate her just exactly as she is. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 218 I well myself off from her, don't hear, don't react. I relax. enjoy, really let go with her. I feel wonderful about being with Believing it's for her own good, I tell her exactly what to do, be, think. I bury my rage and resentment and scurry to appease her to avoid her dis- approval. I approach her with unwarranted, even crazy ideas about her; I don't notice how or if she responds. I go my own separate way. 139. 140. 141. 142. 143. 144. I look after her interests, take steps to protect her. actively back her up. . I freely and openly disclose my innermost self so she truly can know "who I am." ' I am joyful and exuberant and expect to have wonderful fun with her. Just when I am needed most, I abandon her, leave her "in the lurch." I neglect her, her interests, needs. ' I leave her free to do and be whatever she wants. 10. 11. 12. '13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. Subject O User 0 1219 ANSWER SHEET Date Sequence Be sure to use this answer sheet with the questionnaire labeled INTREX FORM Use oencil and completely fill in the circle which best describes your view 0 : Use this ©®®®® ®®®®® ©®©®© ®®©®© NEVER NOT AT ALL scale: 1 @©®©®© @©®©©@ ©®®©®© @©®©®@ ©®®®®®©®©®® ©®®®®®®®©®© ®®®®® ®®©®® ©®®®® ©®©©© ©®®®®®©®©©® ©®®®©®©®©®® ©®®®®®®®©®© ©®®®®®©®©®© ALHAYS PERFECTLY 40 SO 60 7O 85 95 100 A rating of less than 50 indicates "felse"; a rating of 50 or more indicates "true". n®®®®® ©®©®© w®®©®© @®@®@ n®®®®©®®®©©® n@@@®@@@®@@@ RG®®®®®©®©®® w®®®®©©®®®©© fi®®©®©®©®®®® 31'-®@®®@@®®®®G u®®®®® ©®©®® m®©©©© @®©®@ 51~CD®®®®©®®®®© 5L®®®®®G©®®©© 5L®®®®®®©®®®© u®®®® ®©®G®© fi®®©©© ©®®®© w®®®®® ©®®©© n®®©®® $®®®©® ©©®®®® ®©®®®G 57. 58. ©®®®©®©®©®© ®®©®®®©®®®® ©®®®© ®®©©® ®®®®® ®®®®© @eeeee eeeeee eeeeee ®®®©®® ©®®®®®©®®®© ©®®®®®©®©®© eeeee eeeee eeeee eeeee eeeee eeeee @©®@®® @®®©®@ ®®®©®® ©®®©®® ©®®®®© ®@@®®@ fi®®®®©®©®©®® fi®®®®©®©®®©© neeeee @eeee seeeee eeeee meeeeeeeeeee weeeeeeeeeee meeeeeeeeeee weeeeeeeeeee u®©®®© ©®®®© W®®®®© ©®®®© 4s-(06969696969GD®®®® m®®®®©®®®®®© m®®®®®®©®®®© w®®®®®®©®®®® ©®®®®®©®©®® ©®®®®®©®©®© S9. 60. 6L®®®®® ®®®®® ®®®®® ®®®®© ®®®®® ©®®®® 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. m®®®®® w®®®®® 6t®®©®® m®®®®© W®®®®® n®®®®e @©®®®@ ©©®©®® ©®®©®© ©©®©®© ©©®®®© @©G@®© @©®@®® @@®®®@ <5>GD<3>G§I3>GB 6996769533. ®@®@Q® ®@G®?e 76. 77. 78. 79. a 81. 90. 91. 92. 93. -®®®®©©©@®®@ n .®@@@@ Not change to rating : 220 NEVII Use this scale: .®@®@@ ©®©©G -®©®©© ©©®®S -©©®©©©©®©©© ®©®©©®®®®©© ®®®©©©©®®©9 ®©®®©®©®®©© ©®®®©©®®©©© ©®®®©©©®®®® ©©©®©©®®®®© .®®®®©®©®©®© .@®®® ©©®©©© -®©®®@©©®®©© .©©©®© @®©®® ~®®®®® ©®®®® .©©®®©®©®®©® .®©©®@®©@©©© -®®®®©©©©©®© ©©®©J©©®©®G eeeee eeeee eeeee eeeee eeeeeeeeeee eeeeeeeoeee eeeeee NUT KT ALL ALWAYS PERFECTCY 4 s 6 O a rating of less than so indicates “false': a satin; of so or more indicates 'ccue'. sm®®®®© 9L®©®®© sa®®®©© mmGGESE 1u®©®®© 1w®®®®© l%®@®@® 1M®®©®© 1w®®©®© 1w®®®©© 1n®®©®© ©®®©®®ui.®®®®© ©®©©G @©®®©8122.®®®®© @®©©9 ©®®©©®ui®®®@©®©®©©9 ©©®©®®nt®®®®©G©®©@9 ®©®©©©125-®®®® ®©®©®G @gggggue®@@®@@@®@@9 @©®©®©um®®©©© @®@©C @@®©®Gua®®©®© ©®©@G ®®®®©®ut®®®©©®©®©©© ©©®©®®uo®®®®©®®®©©9 @@@@@eun®@@@ ®©®©©G 1W®®®®®©®®©®©uu®©®® ®©®©©© 1w®®®®® 110.636) ®®® 1m®©®®© 1w®©©®© 1m®©®©© ©®®®®®un®©®®© ©®©®® @@®®®®5&®®®®© ©®©®© ©®®®®®3i®3®®©®©®®®© @©®®©©LW®®®®@@©@©@C ©©®®®©Ln®®®®©®©®@@9 114.@©@®@y@©@©©@ 138-®©®®©®©®©©C iw®®®®© 1mG®©®© 117-®©©©® 1m®®®®© 1u®®®®® @©®®®©um®©®@©b©®©®© @©®®®®um®®©®®@©®©©© ®®®©®©LW®®®@©®©®@©© ©®®©®©Lw®@®@®®®@@®@ @®@@wauu@@®®m©©mmma lnme®®®@®@@GGunfireeeweeeee l“ 221 APPENDIX N Introduction to Participant Couples I am Paula C. McNitt of Charleston, Illinois. As part of my work for a doctoral degree in Psychology I am conducting a study of married couples, aged 20 to 60. I am interested in what husbands and wives who are getting along fairly well together, and who are not having serious emotional or relationship problems, have to say about their marriages. I will be asking one or preferable both partners to complete several questionnaires. These should take about one hour to complete. The questions are all pretty straightforward; there's nothing sneaky about them. So, if you fit this description and have not been separated from your spouse very much during the past three months, feel that your marriage has been free of serious problems during the past two years, and if you (and/or your spouse) are willing to participate, please give me your name and telephone number (pass sign up sheet). As a graduate student, I regret that I cannot really pay for your participation; however, I can offer the following to all couples who participate, a "thank. you" of $5.00, a feedback session in which I will review and interpret your questionnaire responses, and my sincere gratitude. l. 222 APPENDIX 0 Survey of Problem During the past two _s_ have you received or sea-imp gm: about ask- ing for professionanfizlrp with any of the following problem? Protessiual help aeans a doctor. psychiatrist. psychologist, social worker. mlor. or sinister. Check all that apply. Castant. severe nervous tnsionuvith luadsches. staunch problns. probleas sleeping. appetite problees. My attacks. this includes hyperventilation palpitatius. choking. shortness of breath, extra-e tension. Extra-e irrational fears tlut keep'you frm doing the things yonnead or want to do-ofear of flying. fear of traveling or leaving the house by yourself. fear of crowds. Mivethoughtsorfeers: thesearethooghts thatyoucalalotcal- trol em though they seen silly. Comlsive. ritual behaviors: things you have to do. cannot help not doing. but which aha little sense. Depressim: ertr-e feelings of sadness. loss of interest in life. sleep and appetite probleas. Alcoholis- or proble- drinking. ,Drugdqodency. Sexual probleas. Marriage probleas. Your child's notional or behavior problens. Other problees getting along with people. Other. Areyoonovtakingaynedicinestohelp'ymfeellessnximorless depressedor to help you to think clearly? If so. which? 3. lhve you ever. since the age of 21. been hospitalized for psychiatric rea- sons? References References Abraham, K. (1960). Notes on the psychoanalytic treatment of manic depressive insanity and allied conditions. In selected papers on psychoanalyses. New York: Basic Books. Akiskal, H. S., & McKinney, T. W. (1975). Overview of recent research in depression: Integration of ten conceptual models into a comprehensive clinical frame. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 285-305. Arieti, S., & Bemporad, J. (1978). Severe and mild depres- sion. New York: Basic Books. Beck, A. T. (1972). Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Benjamin, L. S. (1974). Structural analysis of social behavior. Psychological Review, fig, 392-425. Benjamin, L. S. (1977). Structural analysis of a family in therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, fig, 391-406. Benjamin, L. S. (1979a). A manual for using SASB question- naires to measure correspondence among family historyL self-concept, and current relations with significant others. unpublished manuscript, University of fi—‘— e Wiscon31n. Benjamin, L. S. (1979b). Structural analysis of a differ- entiation failure. Psychiatry, 12, 1—23. von Bertalanffy, L. (1974). General systems theory and psychiatry. In S. Arieti (Ed.) , The American handbook of s chiatr (Vol. 1) (pp. 1095-1122). New York: Basic Books. Bienvenu, M. J. (1974). Measurement of marital communica- tion. The Family Coordinator, 12, 26-31. Bienvenu, M. (1978). A counselor's guide to accompany a marital communication inventory. Saluda, N.C.: Family Life Publications. Bonime, W. (1960). Depression as a practice: Dynamics and psychotherapeutic considerations. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 1, 194-201. 224 Chodoff, P. (1970). The core problem in depression: Inter- personal aspects. Science and Psychoanalysis, ll, 56-61. Chodoff, P. (1974). The depressive personality: A critical review. In R. J. Friedman & M. M. Katz (Eds.), 3‘23 psychology o§__depression: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 55-83). New York: John Wiley and Sons. Cohen, M. 8., Baker, 6., Cohen, R. A., Fromm-Reichman, F., & weigert, E. (1954). An intensive study of twelve cases of manic depressive psychoses. Psychiatry, l1, 103-137. Coleman, R. E. & Miller, A. G. (1975). The relationship between depression and marital maladjustment in a clinic population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- chology,‘4§, 647-651. Coyne, J. C. (1976). Depression and the response of others. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, §§, 186-193. Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry, 32, 28-40. Deykin, E. Y., Jacobson, S., Klerman G. L., & Solomon (1966). The empty nest: Psychological aspects of con- flict between depressed women and their grown children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 1422-1426. Ewing, J. A., Long, V., & Wenzel, G. (1961). Concurrent group psychotherapy of alcoholic patients and their wives. Eternational Journal of Group Psychotherapy, ll, 329-338. Fava, G. A., Kellner, R., Munari, F., Pavan, L., & Pesarin, F. (1982). Losses, hostility, and depression. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170, 474-478. Feldman, L. B. (1976). Depression and marital interaction. Family Process, lg, 389-397. Fiore, A. 8 Swensen, C. H. (1977). Love behavior in func- tional and dysfunctional married couples. Psychological Forrest, T. (1969). The combined use of marital and individual therapy in depressions. Contemporary Psycho- analysis, 2, 76-83. Freud, S. (1959). Mourning and melancolia (orig. 1917). In E. Jones (Ed.), The collected papers of Sigmund Freud. New York: Basic Books. 225 Friedman, A.S. (1970). Hostility, factors and clinical improvement in depressed patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 524-537. Gaylin, W. (1968). The meaning of despair: Psychoanalytic contributions to the understanding of depression. New York: Science House. Gershon, E. S., Cromer, M., & Klerman, G. L. (1968). Hostility and depression. Psyphiatry, 21, 224-235. Haley, J. (1959). The family of the schizophrenic: A model system. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 129, 357- 374. Haley, J. (1963b). Marriage therapy. Archives of General Haley, J. (1963a). Strategies of psychotherapy. New York: Grune and Stratton. Hautzinger, M., Linden, M., & Hoffman, N. (1982). Dis- tressed couples with and without a depressed partner: An analysis of their verbal interaction. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Bxpgrimental Psychiatry, _l3 , 307- 314. Heins, T. (1978). Marital interaction in depression. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatg, 13, 269-275. Hinchliffe, M. R., Hooper, D., & Robert, F. J. (1978). Tag melancholy_marriage: Depression in marriage and psycho- social approaches to therapy. Chichester, England: John Wiley and Sons. Hogan, P. & Hogan, B. K. (1975). The family treatment of depression. In F. Flach & S. Draghi (Eds.), The nature and treatment of depression. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Hollingshead, A, B. (1965). Two-factor index, of social position. Yale University. Hollingshead, A. B. 8 Redlich, F. C. (1958). Social class and mental illness. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Ilfield, F. W. (1977). Current social stressors and symp- toms of depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 161-166. Jackson, D. D. (1957). The question of family homeostasis. The Psychiatric‘guarterly Report, 31, 79-90. 226 Jackson, D. D. (1965). The study of the family. Family Process, 1, 1-20. Jackson, D. D. (1968). Communication, family and marriage: Human Communication (Vol. 1). Palo Alto, California: Science and Behavior Books. Jacobson, S. & Klerman, G. L. (1966). Interpersonal aspects of hospitalized depressed patients' home visits. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 94-102. Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New York: Jason Aronson. Klecka, W. R. (1975). Discriminant analysis. In N. H. Nie, C. H. Hull, J. G. Jenkins, K. Steinbrenner, & D. H. Bent (Eds.), SPSS: Statistical package for the social sciences. New York: McGraw-Hi . Klecka, W. R. (1980). Discriminant analysis. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. Kohl, R. N. (1962). Pathological reaction of marital partners to improvement of patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 118, 1036-1041. Leff, M. J., Roatch, J. F., & Bunney, W. E., Jr. (1970) Environmental factors preceding the onset of severe depressions. Psychiatry, 23, 293-311. Lehman, H. E. (1970). Epidemiology of depressive disorders. In R. R. Fieve (Ed.), Depression in the 1970's modern theory and research. Exerpta Medica, 21-34. Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). A behavioral approach to depres- sion. In R. J. Friedman & M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psy- chology, of depression: Contempogary theory and research. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Lewinsohn, P. & Shaffer, M. (1971). Use of home observa- tion as an integral part of the treatment of depression: A preliminary report and case studies. Journal of Con- sulting and Clinical Psychology, 31, 87-94. Liberman, R. P. & Raskin, D. E. (1971). Depression: A behavioral formulation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 515-523. Libet, J. M. & Lewinsohn, P. M. (1973). Concept of social skill with special reference 11) the behavior of depressed persons. ggprnal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 19, 304-312. 227 McLean, P. D., Ogston, R., & Grauer, L. (1973). A behav- ioral approach to the treatment of depression. Journal prehavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 3, 323- 330. McPartland, T. S. & Hornstra, R. K. (1964). The depressive datum. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 5, 253-261. Murphy, D. C. & Mendelson, L. A. (1973). Communication and adjustment in marriage: Investigating the relationship. Family Process, 13 317-326. Overall, J. (1971). Associations between marital history and the nature of manifest psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 18, 213-221. Paykel, E. 8., Myers, J. K. Dienelt, M. N., Klerman, G. L., Lindenthal, J. J., & Pepper, M. P. (1969). Life events and depression: A controlled study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 753-760. Rashkis, H. (1968). Depression as the manifestation of the family as an open system. Archives of General Psy- Rounsaville, B. J., Prusoff, B. A., & Weissman, M. M. (1980). The course of marital disputes in depressed women: A 48-month follow up. Comprehensive Psychiatry, El, 111-118. Rounsaville, B. J., Weissman, M. M., Prusoff, B. A., & Herceg-Baron, R. L. (1979). Marital disputes and treat- ment outcome in depressed women, 29, 483-490. Rubinstein, D. & Timmons, J. F. (1978). Depressive dyadic and triadic relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family Counseling, 1, 13-23. Rush, A. J., Shaw, B., & Khatami, M. (1980). Cognitive therapy of depression: Utilizing the couples system. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 103-113. Satir, V. (1967). Copjoint family therapy. Palo .Alto, California: Science and Behavior Books. Satir, V. (1971). Symptomatology: A family production. In J. B. Howells (Ed.), Theory and. practice of family psychiatry (pp. 663-670. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Schless, A. P., Mendels, J., Kipperman, A., & Cochrane, C. (1974). Depression and hostility. Journal of Neuroses and Mental Disease, 159, 9l-100. 228 Seligman, M. E. P. (1974). Depression and learned helpless- ness. In R. J. Friedman & M. M. Katz (Eds.), The psy- chology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Spear. D. C. (1970). Family systems: Morphostasis and morphogenesis , or is homeostasis enough? Family Process, 2, 259-278. Spiegel, R. (1965). Communication with depressive patients. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 3, 30-35. Stuart, R. (1967). Casework treatment of depression viewed as an interpersonal disturbance. Social Work, ll, 27- 36. Swensen, C. H. (1961). Love: A self-report analysis with college students. Journal of Individual Psychology, ll, 167-171. Swensen, C. H. (1978). Manual and pest booklet for the scale of feglings and behavior of love. Unpublished manuscript, Purdue University. Swensen, C. H. & Gilner, F. (1968). Scale of feelings and behavior of love . Unpublished manuscript , Purdue University. Tabachnik, N. (1961). Interpersonal relations in suicidal attempts: Some psychodynamic considerations and impli- cations for treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 16-21. Wasli, E. L. M. (1977). Dysfunctional communications response patterns of depressed wives and their husbands in relations to activities of dgily living. Unpublished D.N.Sc. dissertation, the Catholic University’ of America. weissman, M. M. & Klerman, G. L. (1977). Sex differences in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, ll, 98-111. Weissman, M. M. & Paykel, E. S. (1974). The depressed woman: A stud of social relationships. 'Ehicago: The University of Chicago Press. wender, P. (1968). The role of deviation amplifying feed- back in the origin and perpetuation of behavior. Psychiatry, ll, 309-311. 229 Wessman, A. B., Ricks, D. F., & Tyl, M. M. (1960). Char- acteristics and concomitants of mood fluctuation in college women. Journal of .Abnormal and Social Psy- chology, 62, 117-126. Woodruff, R. A., Goodwin, D. W., & Guze, S. (1974). Psychiatric diagnosis. New York: Oxford University Press. Zuckerman, M., Persky, H., Eckman, K. M., & prkins, T. R. (1967). A multitrait multimethod, measurement approach to the traits (or states) of anxiety, depression, and hostility. Journal of Projective Techniques and Per- sonality Assessment, ll, 39-49.