ABSTRACT RECONCEPTUALIZING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ETHNICITY AND PUBLIC RECREATION: A PROPOSED MODEL By Edwin GOmez This study considers the relationship between ethnicity and public recreation settings. A better understanding of the use of public recreation space by minority group members is needed. A more complex measure of the relationships between ethnicity and recreation is needed. This study reviews a selection of the most often cited studies, from the previous 30 years, and outlines the major assumptions and relationships between variables and constructs. Although empirical literature has been growing, there have been (1) methodological inconsistencies, (2) requests for innovative approaches, and (3) requests for research on ethnic groups other than African Americans. Through careful analysis, a more coherent and comprehensive picture of previous research was developed. The end result was the development of a conceptual model which was operationalized in the study. This model is called the Ethnicity and Public Recreation Participation (EPRP) Model and consists of six constructs: (1) acculturation, (2) socioeconomic status, (3) subcultural identity, (4) perceived benefits of public recreation, (5) perceived discrimination, and (6) public recreation participation. Puerto Ricans from Southbridge, Massachusetts represented the ethnic group under study. A questionnaire was developed to measure the PLACE IN REIURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINE return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 020) P U ma mm.“ aforementioned constructs. The EPRP Model was tested empirically with confirmatory factor analysis and path analysis techniques. The initial model was consistent with the data, but error and path coefficient diagnostics led to a revised, theoretically consistent model involving the same constructs, and similar causality. Substantively speaking, there is not much difference between the proposed EPRP Model and the revised EPRP Model. They both hypothesize a similar array of relationships. The researcher de-constructed the model to illustratethe theoretical consistency between parts of the model and earlier models conceptualized by previous researchers. The model was found to be theoretically and empirically sound. The study found that (1) the most important factor was acculturation; (2) there is a significant spurious relationship between the marginality and ethnicity factors; and (3) perceived benefits of recreation is a significant construct to be considered for future studies. Copyright by EDWIN GOMEZ 1 999 DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to my parents: Natividad and Julia Gomez. I can never repay the debt I owe them for their countless sacrifices that allowed me the opportunities to obtain a graduate degree. Through their unconditional love, support, and faith in me I made it through. Los quiero con todo mi corazdn y quiero qua sepan que no son ustedes Ios que deben ser orgulloso de mi, soy yo el que debo ser orgulloso de tenor padres cdmo ustedes. Gracias por todo. iBendicidn! ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to extend very sincere thanks and appreciation to my dissertation committee members: Joe Fridgen for accepting to be the chair of my committee, looking out for my best interests, and serving as a role model; Michael Chubb for sending me constant updates on my topic throughout the two years I spent researching the topic; Dan Stynes for getting me on track earty on with the tough questions regarding my methodology and sample; and John Schweitzer who was my employer and friend for most of my tenure here, and who had countless "talks" with me - professional, statistical, or othenrvise. In addition, I would like to thank five specific individuals that helped immensely through this process: Ralph Levine who met with me off campus and after class to discuss my model and causality; Frank Boster who met with me during the summer, and answered my constant e-mails regarding statistics; Jim Bristor who challenged my writing skills through his editing, and Omara Rivera - Vasquez and Julia Gomez who helped immensely with the translation and formatting of the questionnaires. I would like to thank the many friends I have made at the Department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources, Urban Affairs Programs, and the Department of Psychology at Michigan State University. Thanks for the reality checks and for allowing me to be me. You will all be missed. Lastly, I would like to thank the Puerto Ricans of Southbridge, MA who gave of their time to participate in this study. iGracias por todo! TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................... xii LIST OF FIGURES .............................................. xiii CHAPTER 1 .................................................... 1 INTRODUCTION ................................................ 1 The Statement of the Problem ................................. 5 Objectives ................................................ 7 Hypotheses ............................................... 7 Delimitations .............................................. 8 Definitions ................................................ 9 Basic Assumptions ......................................... 10 Limitations ............................................... 11 CHAPTER 2 ................................................... 12 LITERATURE REVIEW .......................................... 12 Development of the Ethnicity and Marginality Theories ............. 12 The Assumption Regarding Residency ......................... 17 Race and Ethnicity: Is a Distinction Needed Between the Two? ...... 19 Inadequate Measures of Race and Ethnicity ..................... 23 Moving FonNard ........................................... 24 Alternative Approaches to Studying Minority Recreation ............ 25 Construct versus a variable approach. ................... 25 Reconsidering past approaches and exploring alternative theoretical considerations: Pre-cursors to a model ...... 27 CHAPTER 3 ................................................... 45 METHODOLOGY ............................................... 45 Moving Forward: The Development of a Model ................... 45 Questionnaire Design ..................................... 5O Accultu ration. ...................................... 50 Subcultural identity. .................................. 52 Perceived discrimination. ............................. 56 Perceived benefits of recreation. ........................ 57 Socioeconomic status. ............................... 58 Recreation participation. .............................. 59 Subjects and Sampling Frame ................................ 61 Geographic profile. .................................. 62 Demographic profile. ................................. 62 vii Population and sample size. ........................... 64 Procedures for high school sample selection. .............. 65 Procedures for adult population sample selection. .......... 66 Selection of subjects: problems and solutions to creating a list. ................................. 68 Pretesting the Questionnaire ................................. 71 Pretest analysis of scales .............................. 72 Perceived benefits of parks (1A—1G). .................... 73 Public recreation participation (Section 2A-2E). ............ 73 Acculturation scale (Section 3A-3G and 4A-4D). ........... 74 Perceived discrimination (Section 5A-5E). ................ 76 Subcultural identity (Section 6A-6F). ..................... 76 Demographic section of pretest. ........................ 77 Questionnaire distribution. ............................ 78 Collection of Data ........................................ 79 Treatment of Data ......................................... 80 Validity and Reliability ...................................... 81 CHAPTER 4 ................................................... 85 DATA ANALYSIS ............................................... 85 Section One: Descriptive Statistics ............................ 85 Survey Response .................................... 85 Sample and Population Demographics .................... 87 Univariate Analysis .................................... 90 Ancestry. .................................... 90 Acculturation and subcultural identity. .............. 90 Park use. .................................... 92 Data Coding and Clean-up ............................. 95 Section Two: Confirrnatory Factor Analysis ...................... 96 Internal Consistency .................................. 97 Perceived benefits of parks. ...................... 97 Accultu ration. ................................. 99 Perceived discrimination. ....................... 100 Subcultural identity. ........................... 102 Public recreation. ............................. 102 Socioeconomic status. ......................... 103 Test of Parallelism ................................... 103 SES construct as an example of parallelism. ........ 104 Section Three: Path Analysis on EPRP Model .................. 108 Path Analysis Concepts .............................. 109 Over-identification status. ....................... 109 Visualizing the correlation matrix. ................. 110 Local versus global test. ........................ 111 Correlations and path coefficients. ................ 112 The Path Analysis ................................... 113 viii The Revised Model ................................. 118 Path Analysis for Revised Model ........................ 119 The Final Revised Model ............................. 121 Path Analysis for Final Revised EPRP Model .............. 122 Interpretation of the Model ............................ 124 Examination of Hypotheses ........................... 125 Hypotheses .................................. 125 Subcultural identity and socioeconomic status on public recreation usage. ................. 126 Perceived discrimination and perceived benefits on park use. ..................... 126 Subcultural identity and socioeconomic status relationship. ....................... 127 Acculturation/subcultural identity and acculturation/socioeconomic status. .......... 127 Subcultural identity and perceived discrimination. ........................... 127 Summary of Findings ................................ 128 Acculturation, perceived benefits, and perceived discrimination. ........................... 128 Socioeconomic status and subcultural identity. ....... 130 Public recreation participation. .................... 131 Chapter Summary ................................... 131 CHAPTER 5 .................................................. 133 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS ................................. 133 Discussion .............................................. 134 De—constructing the EPRP Model: Reflections of previous literature. ............................. 134 Removal of acculturation. ............................ 134 Removal of perceived benefits. ........................ 136 Removal of acculturation and perceived benefits. .......... 137 Conclusions ............................................. 139 Shortcomings and Limitations ............................... 141 Implications ............................................. 142 Conceptual implications. ............................. 142 Practical implications for the field. ...................... 143 Recommendations for Further Study .......................... 146 Procedural recommendations. ........................ 146 Questionnaire recommendations. ...................... 146 Statistical recommendations. ......................... 147 Conceptual recommendations. ........................ 148 Summary ............................................... 149 ix APPENDIX A ................................................. 150 English and Spanish Questionnaires with Descriptive Statistics .......... 150 Southbridge Public Parks: A Survey of Puerto Rican Residents” Experiences and Opinions ............................ 151 Parques Publicos de Southbridge: Un Estudio de las Experiencias y Opiniones de los Residentes Puertorriquefios ............. 158 APPENDIX B ................................................. 165 Map of Parks in Southbridge and Samples of Correspondence ........... 165 Map of Southbridge, MA and its Public Parks ................... 166 Sample Letter for Community Leaders ........................ 167 Sample of Sign-up Sheet for Survey Participation ................ 168 Sample of Speech for Spanish-speaking Parishioners at St. Mary’s Church During Christmas Eve Mass ..................... 169 Sample Letter for Administration at the High School .............. 170 ' Sample Letter for Administration at Head Start Program ........... 171 Sample Pre-survey Letter: English ........................... 172 Sample Pre-survey Letter: Spanish ........................... 173 Sample Pro-survey Autobiography: English .................... 174 Sample Pre-survey Autobiography: Spanish .................... 175 Sample Introduction Letter for Survey: English .................. 176 Sample Introduction Letter for Survey: Spanish ................. 177 Sample Introduction Letter for Follow-up Survey: English .......... 178 Sample Introduction Letter for Follow-up Survey: Spanish ......... 179 APPENDIX C ................................................. 180 Confirmatory Factor Analysis Concepts ............................. 180 CFA Concepts ........................................... 181 The Measurement Model ................................... 182 Internal Consistency Theorem ............................... 184 Multi—factor Model ........................................ 185 Parallelism Theorem ...................................... 186 Confirmatory Versus Exploratory Factor Analysis ................ 187 APPENDIX D ................................................. 189 Data Analysis Matrix and Chart ................................... 189 Correlation Matrix of 22 Items Used in Final Confinnatory Factor Analysis ...................... 190 Local and Global Tests for Three EPRP Models ................. 191 REFERENCES ................................................ 192 References ............................................. 193 Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. Table 6. Table 7. Table 8. Table 9. Table 10. Table 11. Table 12. Table 13. Table 14. Table 15. Table 16. Table 17. Table 18. Table 19. Table 20. LIST OF TABLES ConceptsNariables Addressed in the Literature Regarding Ethnicity/Race and Public Recreation Participation ..... 26 Direct and Indirect Effects of Ethnicity Constructs on Recreation Participation .............. 49 The Acculturation Scale ............................... 53 Subcultural Identity Scale .............................. 55 Perceived Discrimination. .............................. 57 Scoring of Park Use for Each Park ....................... 61 Mailed Surveys and Response .......................... 86 Age and Gender Breakdown in Census and Sample ......... _ 88 Education for Persons 25 and Over of Latino Origin in Census and Sample ..................... 89 ANOVA of Years in Puerto Rico by Age Cohort ............. 91 Mean Difference in Perceived Benefits of Parks Between Students and Adults .............. 93 Assessing Errors for BEN Construct ...................... 98 Descriptive Statistics For Items Used in the CFA ........... 101 Item by Factor and Factor by Factor Correlation Matrix ...... 105 Parallelism Analysis ................................. 107 Reliability Analysis .................................. 108 Obtained Correlation Matrix for Path Analysis (N=318) ...... 113 Matrices for Original EPRP Model from Program Path ....... 116 Matrices for EPRP Model from Program Path (revised) ...... 120 Matrices for EPRP Model from Program Path (final version) . . 123 xi Figure 1. Figure 2. Figure 3. Figure 4. Figure 5. Figure 6. Figure 7. Figure 8. Figure 9. Figure 10. Figure 11. Figure 12. Figure 13. Figure 14. Figure 15. Figure 16. Figure 17. Figure 19. Figure 20. Figure 21. LIST OF FIGURES Lindsay & Ogle’s (1972) Conceptual Model ................ 28 Craig’s (1972) Conceptual Model ........................ 30 Washbume’s (1978) Conceptual Model ................... 31 Klobus-Edwards' (1981) Conceptual Model ................ 32 Stamps 8. Stamps’ (1985) Conceptual Model ............... 34 Hutchison’s (1987) Conceptual Model .................... 35 West’s (1989) Conceptual Model ........................ 37 A Model of the Factors Affecting Ethnic Leisure Participation . . . 39 Conceptual Model of Explanations of Ethnic Variation in Recreational Behavior ......................... 42 The Ethnicity and Public Recreation Participation Model ...... 45 The EPRP Model (with non-constrained paths) ............ 111 The EPRP Model (revised) ............................ 119 The EPRP Model (final version) ........................ 122 The EPRP Model (final version) ........................ 124 The EPRP Model without Acculturation .................. 135 The EPRP Model without Perceived Recreation Benefits ..... 137 The EPRP Model without ACC and BEN ................. 138 One Factor Measurement Model ........................ 182 Multi-Factor Measurement Model ....................... 185 EFA Model ........................................ 188 xii Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Over the past twenty years, research regarding minorities and public recreation participation has predominantly centered around two theories: (1) ethnicity theory, and (2) marginality theory. The ethnicity theory simply states that minorities do not participate in ”mainstream" recreation activities due to cultural differences. The marginality theory states that participation in recreation activities by minorities is limited due to socioeconomic factors. A Although studies regarding ethnicity, socioeconomic behavior, and recreation have existed prior to 1978 (Burch, 1969; Hendee, 1969; Craig, 1972; Lindsay & Ogle, 1972; Schumen 8. Gmenbeng, 1972), it was Washbume’s (1978) seminal study that initiated the current paradigm regarding the ethnicity and marginality theories’ approach to studying ethnic/racial phenomenon with respect to recreation and recreation settings. Since then, problems have occurred in operationalizing major concepts, and consequently, in measuring the two theories. I A decade after the marginality/ethnicity framework was developed, West (1989) made a significant conceptual contribution to the empirical literature by noting that recreation researchers neglected to explore the problem of interracial relations. Discrimination (whether real or perceived) became a probable explanation for the under representation of minority groups in outdoor recreation settings. The inclusion of this concept creates an even more complex and sensitive framework for the study of minorities and recreation behavior. In the 1990s, the empirical literature regarding ethnicity/race and recreation behavior has been growing in importance for several practical and empirical reasons. One practical reason is changing population demographics (Dwyer 8. Gobster, 1992). Ethnic group members have increased tremendously as a percentage of the population in the United States (US). A culturally (ethnically and racially) diverse population is projected to increase. Dwyer (1994) reported that between 1990 and 2025, the US. population is expected to increase by 50 million and that 81% of the increase will be in minority groups. According to the US. Bureau of the Census, by the year 2010, Hispanics are projected to be the dominant ethnic group, surpassing Blacks. As a result of demographic changes, a related reason for the study of ethnicity and recreation is that recreation providers will have tremendous challenges ahead in terms of service delivery, policy-making, and identifying participation patterns of "non-traditional” users. A better understanding of the use of public recreation space by minority group members is needed. Blacks and Hispanics, the two largest groups, tend to concentrate in urban centers. This concentration of minorities in cities could mean declining use of state and national parks and added stress to urban park and recreation resources. In addition to the practical reasons for the significance of a study on minorities and their recreation behavior, there are several empirical reasons to conduct such studies. First, the relationship between one's ethnicity/race and park use is unclear from previous literature and requires further exploration and/or reconceptualization. Second, conceptually, recreation researchers have had difficulty in defining and operationalizing key concepts when researching ethnicity and recreation. Third, the nature of the interaction between ethnicity and marginality requires further exploration. Current understanding of the two concepts and their combined impact on recreation behavior has not been specifically explored in the design of previous studies. Fourth, and most important (conceptually), Floyd, Gramman and Saenz (1993) and West (1989) noted that marginality and ethnicity theories have suggested that a perception of discrimination influences levels of recreation participation. However, "lack of attention paid to this issue in the recreation research literature continues despite current policy debates regarding structural discrimination and the spate of empirical research on discrimination in the social sciences at large“ (Floyd, et al., 1993). Research is needed to explain the impact discrimination has on use of recreation areas. Although the empirical literature has been growing, several concerns have been identified: (1) methodological inconsistencies, (2) requests for innovative approaches, and (3) requests for research on minority groups other than African Americans (whom are the dominant minority group used for comparison). In response to the first concern, this study reviews a selection of previous studies and outlines the major assumptions and relationships between various variables and constmcts. Through careful analysis, a more coherent and comprehensive picture of previous research will be developed. An innovative approach to research involving minorities and recreation behavior is the measurement of one of the independent variables - subcultural identity. This approach differs from previous studies because it is not simply identifying persons as Black, White, or Hispanic, but rather it asks the extent to which one feels they are affiliated as a member of a minority group. The constmct is a more productive measure of ethnicity. This reconceptualization is important for two reasons. First, it is more consistent with Washbume's original conception of the ethnicity theory. Second, it focuses less on the issue of whether the minority group is ethnic or racial, and focuses more on the issue of minority versus majority. The subcultural identity dimension measures one’s identity as a minority group member, no matter the racial or ethnic makeup. This notion detects the identification with the minority culture versus the majority culture. Although this is different from the traditional sociological conception of race and ethnicity, it is consistent with everyday usage. For example, when majority people discriminate against a minority group member, they often do not distinguish between whether the minority is a racial minority or an ethnic minority. The literature review provides an additional innovation - a basis for the development of a conceptual model that considers the interaction of several ideas regarding ethnicity and recreation behavior. After reviewing past studies, the author has incorporated several conceptual constnicts into a model illustrating the interactions between the different constructs. This model consolidates previous research into a ”snapshot" of what the empirical literature considers to be the most critical dimensions of ethnicity, recreation behavior. Park use is used as a proxy for recreation behavior in this study. This study also considers the request for research on minority groups other than Blacks. The Hispanic pepulation is the second largest minority population in the United States. Of these, Mexicans have been looked at in particular in recreation studies because they are the largest component of the Hispanic minority group. Previous studies that researched Hispanics are often concentrated in the Midwest, South or West. This study differs from previous studies in that the comparison ethnic group consists of Puerto Ricans living in the Northeast. In addition, most of the Puerto Ricans in Southbridge are immigrants (respondents emigrated from Puerto Rico) or first generation Americans (parents from Puerto Rico, but respondents born in continental US). This allows for the inclusion of assimilation/acculturation measures in the analysis of recreation behavior. The Statement of the Problem The purpose of this study is to identify the factors (based on empirical literature and logic) which are most important in explaining the relationship between one’s (sub)cultural identity and its effect on one’s recreation behavior as represented by park use (dependent variable). These (independent) factors include the following: (1) acculturation, (2) socioeconomic status, (3) subcultural identity (minority versus majority), (4) perceived park benefits, (5) perceived discrimination. Presently the research question is no longer whether ethnicity (subcultural identity) is a stronger predictor of recreation behavior than marginality (socioeconomic status), or vice versa. The issue is unresolved, but research is leaning towards ethnicity as the greater predictor of recreation behavior. As such, this study considers three research questions. The first research question addresses which of the two factors (marginality or ethnicity) is the better predictor of recreation behavior. The second research question addresses the combined importance of ethnicity and marginality factors on recreation behavior (park use). The third research question considers the relationships between acculturation, perceived benefits, and perceived discrimination on park use. The development of a model will illustrate the relationships between the previously identified factors. These relationships are based on prior empirical results. The purpose of the model is to simplify the literature and provide a basis for conceptually thinking about minority issues related to recreation. The model is not exhaustive, rather, it reflects the important "currents" in the literature. For example, there could be other factors that could influence park use, such as distance to the resource, cost of transportation, age, and family life cycle stage. However, exploration of these and other factors are not possible due to time, financial, and practical constraints. Obiggiveg 1. Determine the relative influence of subcultural identity and socioeconomic status on park use. Determine the relative influence of acculturation, socioeconomic status, subcultural identity, benefits sought and perceived discrimination on park use. Develop a model to illustrate the relationships between the different factors influencing park use. Hymtheses 1. Subcultural identity [SID] and socioeconomic status [SES] are significant predictors of public recreation participation [REC]. Subcultural identity will be a stronger predictor of park use than socioeconomic status. Ho: rsroIRrsc = 0. l'srssrRrsc = 0 H1: rsrorrzrsc * 0- rsrssrrzec * 0 H23 l'srornrsc > rSESIREC There is a significant relationship between perceived discrimination [DIS] and public recreation participation, and perceived benefits of recreation [BEN] and public recreation participation. H03 rDIS/REC = 0. raswnec = 0 H13 Isle/Rec 9‘ 0. rsrssrRrsc i" 0 There is a significant relationship between the subcultural identity factor [SID] and socioeconomic status [SES]. I"Io: rsro/srss = 0 H13 rsrorses * 0 There is a significant negative relationship between acculturation [ACC] and subcultural identity. There is a significant positive relationship between acculturation and socioeconomic status. Ho: rACCISID Z 0- I'rtccrsrss 5 0 H13 rACCISID < 0. rAce/ses > 0 5. There is a positive relationship between subcultural identity and perceived discrimination. Ho: r00 5 0 H1: rCD > 0 6. There is a negative relationship between perceived discrimination [DIS] and public recreation participation [REC]. Ho: rmc z 0 H13 romeo ‘ 0 7. The path model hypothesis: Ho: the data are consistent with the model (Error = 0). H,: the data are inconsistent with the model (Error :0). Delimitations There are several things that could be included in this study regarding minorities and recreation behavior. For example, one could include data regarding life-cycle, the presence of children, neighborhood ethnic/racial mix, or sense of belonging to a community. Structural factors such as the impact that schools have on minorities and recreation behavior could also be considered. Anthropological factors such as the historical development of the ethnic make up of Southbridge and intercultural relations would also be noteworthy. While some of the factors previously mentioned will be considered in the literature review, as well as in the background of the city, it will be contextual in nature. To study all possible and relevant factors would be both time-consuming and costly. This study is not meant to be comprehensive, but rather it converges upon the key variables outlined in the literature review. The study is further delimited to one city: Southbridge, Massachusetts. All cities have a very unique sociocultural history. As such, comparisons will vary from city to city. Although distinct findings will vary by city, a clearer understanding can be made possible regarding the Puerto Rican population because of the close ties that Southbridge Puerto Ricans maintain with the native population in Puerto Rico. Definitions Cultural Acculturation: the process whereby ethnic and racial minority groups retain their own cultural norms while adopting aspects of the dominant culture. Cultural Assimilation: the process whereby ethnic and racial minority groups absorb the cultural and behavioral patterns of the dominant group (Gordon, 1 964). Cultural Identification: the extent to which one identifies with the minority (subcultural) or majority (mainstream) culture. Ethnic Minorities: minorities who are discriminated against by the dominant group because of cultural differences (language, customs, traditions). Mainstreamed Values: values that are consistent with norms established by the majority group. When one is "mainstreamed" it connotes a weaker affiliation with a minority culture and a stronger affiliation with the majority culture. Mainstreaming is the process by which one is integrated into the wider socioculture. Majority Group: defined as the dominant group in a cultural setting that sets the cultural norm(s) for their given society. In the United States, traditionally, this dominant group has been Whites - males in particular. Minority Group: defined as the ethnic or racial groups which ascribe to subcultural norms within the wider socioculture. Racial Minorities: minorities who are discriminated against by the dominant group because of their phenotype (physical features). Subculture; the use of subculture in this study connotes a minority culture which is different from the dominant culture. It is not meant to imply that a minority culture has less value than the dominant culture. Basic Assumptions It is assumed that individuals have basic values that are particular to their cultural environment. The family is the strongest influence on one’s value system and cultural norms. This value system guides behavior, overall, and recreation behavior, in particular. There are no private parks in Southbridge, MA. Although public parks are located around neighborhood areas, they are also located off of major city streets. There are also no restrictions to public park use by residents in other parts of the city. As such, it is assumed that all Southbridge residents have access to all Southbridge public parks. It is assumed that there is a dominant paradigm for recreation in the United States. This paradigm is reinforced by the White majority. The stronger 1O one identifies with the dominant culture the more they will participate in public recreation activities and settings. Limfis’ tions The Puerto Rican respondents in this study cannot be considered representative of other Hispanic groups and should not be generalized to the entire Hispanic population. Southbridge Puerto Ricans can be generalized to other Puerto Rican populations in the US. mainland and the island of Puerto Rico. Although not representative of the entire Hispanic population, Puerto Ricans can be compared to other Hispanic groups to illustrate differences and similarities between the many Hispanic group members. 11 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Washbume's 1978 seminal study initiated the current paradigm regarding the ethnicity and marginality theories’ approach to studying ethnic/racial phenomenon with respect to recreation settings. Since then, problems have occurred in operationalizing major concepts, and consequently, in measuring the two theories. As such, this chapter provides a context for the development of both the marginality and ethnicity theories. This chapter also provides a critique of the literature regarding ethnicity/race and recreation and alternative F approaches for studying the topic. Development of the Ethnicmr' and Marginal'sy Theories Washbume (1978) provided recreation researchers with the conceptual definitions for ethnicity and marginality (within the context of Black underparticipation in wildland recreation). “The marginality perspective [suggests] that Blacks do not participate because of poverty and various consequences of socioeconomic discrimination ..." (Washbume, 1978, p. 176). As an alternative explanation to the marginality perspective, Washbume maintained " that leisure patterns of Blacks are based on their subcultural style, or ethnicity (Md, p. 177). Note that the original definition of ethnicity referred to "subcultural style." Consequent studies treated ethnicity as synonymous with race or ethnic group designation. This is a deviation from Washbume’s initial conception of ethnicity as subcultural style, and has different meanings in the wider social science 12 literature (West, 1989). The implication is subtle. The focus should not be on the ethnicity of particular persons, but rather the identification that people have towards a culture which is different from the dominant culture. Sex, age, education, and income were the variables used to measure socioeconomic status in Washbume’s study. Blacks and Whites were compared. However, the ethnicity perspective was not examined, only the marginality perspective. Washbume did not consider researching the ethnicity perspective on the grounds that there was inadequate data suitable "for a realistic examination of ethnicity as a valid perspective" (1978, p. 185). Washbume found that Blacks are constrained by marginality factors. However, he also mentioned that leisure differences between Blacks and Whites " are not solely attributable to socioeconomic factors, " and later forecasted "declining utility for the marginality perspective” (1978, p. 179). The implications of his findings were ambiguous at best. While firmly establishing a logical argument for the ethnicity and marginality theories, his conclusions were inconclusive. This set the stage for consequent studies in the 1980s. Researchers in the 1980s (Hutchison, 1987; Klobus-Edwards, 1981; Stamps & Stamps, 1985; West, 1989; and Woodard, 1988) utilized the marginality and ethnicity theories and designed their studies to answer the question of which one of the two theories was correct. Various researchers provided support and mixed results for both theories. Support for the ethnicity theory came from Klobus—Edwards (1981) and Stamps and Stamps (1985). Support for the marginality theory came from Woodard (1988) and West (1989). 13 Mixed results regarding support for marginality and ethnicity came from Hutchison (1987). I In the latter part of the 1980s, and particularly in the eariy 1990s, recreation researchers heralded a new era regarding research on minority groups and recreation settings. The focus was altered from determining which one of the two theories is correct to "accepting” either one of the two theories, or embracing a combination of the two theories and concentrating on other matters. These "other“ matters included the following: . utilization of other minority groups that were not Black for points of comparison (Chavez, 1993; Hutchison, 1987; Floyd, Gramman & Saenz, 1993); . creation of projection models, use of demographics, and the determination of future demand (Christensen & Dwyer, 1994; Dwyer, 1993, 1994; Murdock, Backman, Hoque 8. Ellis, 1991; Murdock, Backman, Colberg, Hoque & Hamm, 1990); . use of observational techniques in the research (Hutchison, 1987); . critique of the literature, as well as general reviews were more prominent (Allison, 1988; Dwyer & Gobster, 1992; Gramman, 1996; Hutchison, 1988; Johnson, Bowker, English & Worthen, 1997); . definitions of race and ethnicity were debated upon (Hutchison, 1988); . identification of other factors such as discrimination, family generations, and neighborhood residence (Phillip, 1994, 1997; Taylor, 1992; West, 1989); . and the corroboration of subjective/qualitative measures and objective/quantitative measures (Allison, 1988; Carr 8 Williams, 1993a; Floyd, et al., 1994; Woodard, 1988). Throughout the various studies noted above, marginality and ethnicity have been measured in different ways. The problem with the different approaches to measurement is that they have not allowed for true comparisons between 14 studies because of two things: (1) different methodological approaches, and (2) different operationalization of the same variables. Variables measuring marginality have been approached very differently from Hutchison’s eariier conception. Woodard (1988), for example, looked at marginality in terms of 'classism,” which (citing E. Franklin Frazier) he defines as "the belief that one should only recreate with one’s own social class group" (p. 94). Floyd, et al.’s, (1994) approach to marginality treated social class in "subjective terms" (p. 162). The variables measuring ethnicity were consequently divided into race variables and ethnic variables (recall that this is a deviation from earlier studies lumping ethnic and racial groups under one category). Taylor (1992) looked at ethnic expression and participation in ethnic leisure activities as measures of ethnicity. Carr and Williams (1993b) considered ethnicity in terms of three dimensions: (1) ancestral origin, (2) generational status, and (3) levels of acculturation. Another example of operational inadequacies is residency. Klobus- Edwards (1981) used residency to determine whether respondents lived (resided) in Black or racially mixed neighborhoods. West’s (1989) approach provided a spatial/regional context, while Taylor (1992) defined residency as the amount of time the respondent has lived in the city. In addition to variable operationalization issues, Meeker, Woods & Lucas (1973) mentioned the effect that past injustices and discrimination had on Blacks’ recreational behavior. West (1989) noted that discrimination was not 15 looked at as a variable affecting use of public spaces by minorities (Blacks in particular). Discrimination, therefore, was added as a theoretical explanation for minority underparticipation in leisure settings: These two paradigms - marginality and subculture theory - have dominated the thinking and research about minority underrepresentation in outdoor recreation. However, it is a strange irony that this body of research has almost entirely ignored another important potential explanation: the problem of interracial relations and prejudice (West, 1989, p. 12). West uses "subcultural theory” in lieu of ethnicity theory because he believed that Washbume’s initial conception and theoretical assumptions (regarding ethnicity) were closer to the conception of subcultural life preferences. West’s measure of subcultural preference was operationalized as "no interest“ or “prefer to do other things”. In other words, preference or non-preference indicated ethnicity factors. However, West noted that the "measures of subcultural influence [were] less robust than other measures” (1989, p. 17). This indicates that a more complex measure of cultural preference is needed. Although West did measure discrimination, he did not consider the possibility of an interaction between subculture and marginality. Conversely, Hutchison (1987) mentioned that Black and White differences could be due to a complex interaction between race and class, but he did not measure the interaction, nor did he consider the role of discrimination. Lack of attention on the nature of these relationships is due to a confluence of assumptions underlying the research variables, an ahistorical approach to dealing with race and ethnicity, and inadequate measures of the concepts under study. 16 The As um tion R a in R si no Regardless of how residency has been operationalized in previous studies (see earlier note), Stamps and Stamps” (1985) assumption that residential integration facilitates social integration has influenced the literature on minorities and recreation. The assumption states that Blacks who live in White or racially mixed neighborhoods will have similar recreation behaviors as Whites. Although this assumption has existed prior to Stamps and Stamps’ mention of it, it has created two problems. The first problem is that it assumes a normative order of White recreation. Therefore, if subcultural groups are not participating in the normative order, they are considered to be under- represented. Johnson, et al. (1997), mentioned that "Woodard (1988) and Carr and Williams (1993) [argued] that subgroup recreation should be considered in terms of the respective ethnic group standards rather than in relation to a normalized ideal" (p. 4). The second problem is that the assumption (of equating residential integration with social integration) is contradictory to other studies specifically researching the issue or racially mixed neighborhoods. From the Detroit Area Study of 1978, Whites and Blacks were asked questions concerning neighborhood racial composition: it [became] very clear that whites still harbor substantial prejudice against blacks as potential neighbors, and that their tolerance for racial mixing is really quite limited This fundamental disparity between blacks and whites has been confirmed by surveys conducted [in 1991] in Milwaukee, Omaha, Cincinnati, Kansas City, and Los Angeles, all of which show that blacks strongly prefer a 50-50 mixture and that whites have little tolerance for racial mixture beyond 20% black (Massey & Danton, 1993, pg 93). 17 Furthermore, simply because Blacks are in a racially mixed neighborhood, it does not signify that they are particularly welcomed. Historically, so-called "neighborhood associations" became popular in the 19603. Their main purpose was to keep blacks out of the neighborhoods. By 1965, Detroit alone had "at least 192 neighborhood organizations throughout the city,” founded by Whites (Sugrue, 1996, p. 211). This could hardly be called welcoming. Perceptions and feelings towards Blacks, because of these organizations, instill values for the next generation, thereby perpetuating a cycle of racial discrimination and hostility. Perhaps a more appropriate measure of how well Blacks and Whites live together is their sense of belonging on the block. McMillan and Chavis (1986) identified "membership” as a critical element or criteria for inclusion (both in the neighborhood setting, specifically and in society, generally). Rather than inferring from the mixture of the neighborhood (is a respondent in a racially mixed neighbomood or in a predominantly Black or White neighborhood), one should consider asking the respondent whether or not they feel they belong. If there is a strong sense of belonging among minority members of the neighborhood, than minorities are more likely to use surrounding neighborhood parks and participate in those public parks. There are other reasons why sense of belonging in a block is a better measure than place of residence. In a neighborhood with a strong sense of belonging, neighbors are more apt to interact and know each other. This leads to knowing people on the block and less crime because people are more likely to 18 watch out for each other. Less crime would allow people to interact more in public places, such as nearby parks. Bag sng Ethnic'fly: Is a Distinction Neadfi Between the Two? In the recreation literature, perhaps the most critical argument that has surfaced is the need for distinction between race and ethnicity. Hutchison’s (1988) critique of the race/ethnicity literature regarding recreation research was an excellent exposition on the inadequacies of the marginality and ethnicity perspectives. In particular, he noted that race and ethnicity "have been used interchangeably without careful definition of either concept, even though the implied association between race and ethnicity contradicts research in race and ethnic relations“ (Hutchison, 1988, p. 11). Hutchison, a sociologist by training, makes a critical observation: some recreation researchers are ahistorical in that they do not research enough of the wealth of information available on race relations in the sociological literature to provide a proper context. However, the question which needs to be asked is: does it matter? The answer is yes and no. It matters that recreation researchers familiarize themselves with such things as Jim Crow laws, (racial) restrictive covenants, and developments in federal and municipal policy which helped to reinforce racial cleavages in urban settings. 'By 1970, after two decades of urban renewal, public housing projects in most large cities had become black reservations, highly segregated from the rest of society and characterized by extreme social isolation" (Massey & Danton, 1993, p. 57). Given this pattern, one has to question whether or not Blacks have similar social patterns as compared to 19 Whites. Having a sense of the history of race relations might provide some insight into why Blacks don’t want to participate in the same public recreation space as Whites. Race relations has formally been studied since the late nineteenth century. While it’s initial focus was on Black and White relations, it has expanded its scope to include other minority groups. A cursory glance at the vast race relations literature will show how sociologists have changed their thinking about the race ”problem." Blacks were initially looked at as biologically inferior to Whites. Much of the earlier work was influenced by the Eugenics Movement (this movement espoused the theory that the human species can be improved by controlling for heredity) and Social Darwanism. At the turn of the century, sociologists began to look at blacks as culturally inferior when biological arguments began waning. From the 1920s, Robert Park and his students from the Chicago School of Sociology legitimated “the idea that assimilation [italics added] was the inevitable end point of race relations” (McKee, 1993, p. 110). Race “problems“ turned to race ”relations.” After World War II, Gunnar Myrdal’s An Amsn’ssn Dilsmms transformed race relations into a moral issue. Myrdal chose to title his voluminous work "An American Dilemma” because he felt it captumd ”his sense of the race problem in the United States: a conflict between the highest of American ideals and lower parochial interests and prejudices” (McKee, 1993, p. 226). 20 In particular, Myrdal noted that it was not so much a black man’s problem as it was a white man’s problem. Myrdal, however, still supported the assimilationist theory. It wasn’t until the post civil-rights era that this assimilationist perspective began to change into a pluralistic, multicultural perspective of race relations; the reasons for which are beyond the scope of this study. As mentioned eariiar, it is important that leisure scientists understand the development of the race ”issue," and put it in its proper context when researching racial/ethnic matters. However, it is not as important to distinguish between race and ethnicity for empirical results to be relevant. Race groups are defined, in the sociological sense, in terms of their phenotype (e.g., hair texture, skin color, and facial features). Ethnic groups are defined as identifying with a particular culture (real or imagined). Although Hutchison (1988) points out that, historically, there is a difference between the two terms, he failed to note that within the discipline of sociology, there are two schools of thought on this issue. Theorists who have compared race and ethnicity may be divided into two camps: those who underscore the likeness of the concepts and those who view them as sufficiently dissimilar to warrant distinct perspectives and policies. To proponents of racial and ethnic similarity, distinction between the two concepts are frequently seen as based on definitional differentiation. In other words, distinguishing between the two concepts has validity only as an exercise in differentiating between what are merely theoretical constructs. Distinctions between Race and Ethnicity therefore, it is argued, are untenable and without empirical support (Singh, 1981, p.2). This supports the view that racial and ethnic groups could be viewed together. While Blacks (a racial minority group) are different from other (ethnic) 21 minority groups, given their slavery past, they are similar to other minority groups in that they are marginalized (economically, politically, and socially) in some fashion. Again, the question is: does it matter? This study contends that it is much less important to stress the difference between race and ethnicity (unless one is researching specifically a race versus ethnicity issue), than it is to stress the importance of the difference between minority (marginalized) groups and majority (dominant) groups: Whites and non- Whites. Given current demographic changes, identified by Dwyer (1993), it will be increasingly important to look at minority groups (regardless of racial or ethnic makeup) and how they differ from mainstream (majority) America, with respect to recreation behavior. The central concern, therefore, should not be one of definition, but rather one of applicability and practicality. Following this argument, one has to question whether or not Blacks have their own culture, which would make them both a racial and an ethnic group by current definitions. Singh (1981) comments that ethnic groups "may share a similar language, religion, and/or history" (p. 1). Residential segregation and social isolation has caused Black speech patterns to differ from Standard American English. [Black English] evolved independently from Standard American English because blacks were historically separated from whites by caste, class, and region [Black English] has become progressively more uniform across urban areas. Over the past two decades, the Black English vemaculars of Boston, Chicago, Detroit, New York, and Philadelphia have become increasingly similar in their grammatical stmcture and lexicon, reflecting urban blacks’ common social and economic isolation within urban America (Massey 8. Danton, 1993, pp. 162-163). 22 The ramifications of a separate dialect are tremendous. If Black English is uniform and separate, it creates barriers to communication between Blacks and Whites, thereby increasing less interaction; even in a racially mixed neighborhood. Furthermore, it impedes Blacks’ socioeconomic progress because it will be harder for Blacks to find a job if they do not speak Standard English well. Wilson (1996) noted that "employers frequently mentioned concerns about [Black] applicants' language skills and educational training" (p. 116). It will also be more difficult for Blacks to do well in school when they have to read texts that are in Standard English. It has been found that successful Blacks “who have grown up in the ghetto literally become bilingual, Ieaming to switch back and forth between black and white dialects depending on the social context" (Massey & Danton, 1993, p. 165). In addition to language, Blacks share a similar history (a.g. slavery) and a similar religion (e.g. spirituals). lnadsggsts Mfisugs sf Rsce and Ethnicmr' Given the above discussion, it is easy to see why the categorical scale is inadequate for measuring a complex variable such as ethnicity or race. Race and ethnic categories have been too limiting. Although recreation researchers have classified respondents as Black, White, or Hispanic, they do not know the extent to which the respondents (pertaining to a particular category) identify with their respective culture(s). For example, how afrocentric is the Black respondent? 'Afrocentricity espouses the belief that images and symbols derived from African and African American life experiences are necessary and appropriate guides for people of African descent” (West, 1994, p. 28). 23 Therefore, the more afrocentric Blacks are, the more likely they are to be exposed to African American literature, music, history, and dance. Conversely, the less afrocentric Blacks are, the more likely they are to reflect mainstream values and participate in mainstream pursuits. A more complicated measure of cultural identification (versus a categorical approach) is needed in order to gauge the extent to which respondents identify with their respective cultures. Movi_ngForwa_rd The Jsumal of Leismre Research dedicated an entire issue (1998, volume 30) to minority recreation and leisure. This special issue is the most current collection providing a critique of previous research. Of particular interest is the introduction by Myron F. Floyd and the article by Karla Henderson. Floyd (1998) noted: Perhaps the most critical issue currently facing the race and ethnic studies literature is the absence of viable theoretical frameworks. In general, little systematic thought has been directed at race and ethnic issues three theoretical explanations have historically been used the marginality hypothesis, the ethnicity or subcultural hypothesis, and perceived discrimination scholarship on race, ethnicity and leisure will advance if past approaches are reconsidered and alternative theoretical and methodological possibilities are explored (pp. 4-5). Henderson (1998) noted: One of the problems researchers encounter is the tendency to examine subjects or participants as simply one characteristic when all of us are more complex than simply being female or white or able-bodied (p. 159). The suggestion by both researchers is that there is a need for (1) more complex forms of measurement, and (2) an integrative approach to reflect the interrelationships between these different dimensions. 24 Alternative A ches to tud in Mino ' Recreation Qpnstrug VSEQS a variable approach. A call for new approaches has been issued by researchers. One such approach is to look at a construct (conceptual) approach versus a variable approach in order to understand the relationships between subcultural differences and public recreation participation. In order to synthesize past research, this investigation looked at the variables which were used to measure particular phenomena related to ethnic recreation. In addition to previously used variables, underlying constructs are normally mentioned by researchers (or assumed) but never specifically addressed. Table 1 illustrates major concepts and variables which were addressed in the 1980s and 19903. From Table 1, one can see the several concepts which were researched. However, these concepts and variables were typically measured so that one can see the nature of the relationship among (unidimensional) variables and the dependent variable: participation. The problem with this approach is that it does not take into account influences which the independent variables exude on each other, Le, a multidimensional approach. As a result, researchers often fall short of understanding or identifying a minority recreation phenomenon. 25 Table 1 ConceptsNariables Addressed in the Literature Regarding Ethnicig/Race and Publis Recreation Participation ConceptsNariables Researchers (Year) (Approach to Concept) Marginality Klobus—Edwards (1981) Stamps & Stamps (1985) Woodard (1988) (classism) West (1989) Taylor (1992) Floyd, et al. (1994) (subjective) Race/Ethnicity Klobus—Edwards (1981) Stamps 8 Stamps (1985) Hutchison (1987) West (1 989) Taylor (1992) (ethnic identification) Carr & Williams (1993) (3 dimensions of ethnicity) Chavez (1993) Floyd, et al. (1993) (ethnic identification) Floyd, at al. (1994) (race) Sex/Gender Edwards (1981) Woodard (1989) Taylor (1992) Residency Edwards (1981) (black or mixed neighborhoods) West (1989) (spatial regional context) Taylor (1992) (time living in city) Age Woodard (1988) Chavez (1993) Floyd, et al. (1993) Discrimination Woodard (1988) West (1 989) Chavez (1993) 26 Reconsidering Past Approachessnd ExplonligAltemstive Theoreticail @nsiderations: Pre-cursors to a Model. This section identifies the undenying dimensions (whether measured or implied) of previous research. A critique of assumptions and their consequences will also be offered when necessary. This section is not meant to be exhaustive. As such, selective studies which have influenced the scholarship of racial/ethnic recreation research will be considered for review. The studies are considered chronologically to illustrate development of scholarship. Lastly, conceptual models are provided to identify constmcts from past research. The reader should keep in mind that these conceptual models have been created by the current investigator, not by previous researchers, for the purpose of illustrating relationships between said constmcts. Prior to Washbume’s (1978) study, Lindsay and Ogle (1972) and Craig (1972) studied Black recreation patterns in relation to urban areas. Craig identified the distance/cost factor as a major impediment to Blacks’ leisure. Lindsay and Ogle mentioned physical availability as a factor, in addition to cost, and introduced the opportunity theory (a precursor to marginality theory). Opportunity theory (as cited from Hendee) was defined by Lindsay and Ogle as suggesting “that groups long denied opportunities, not only by virtue of their residence, but because of poverty, ignorance or segregation, might become participants in available opportunities if these barriers are removed” (1972, p. 20). Lindsay and Ogle believed that if one were to control for cost, than there should be no difference in opportunities between low and high income groups, 27 and participation in recreation activities would be equal. Their conceptual model is reflected in Figure 1. Figure 1. Lindsay & Ogle’s (1972) Conceptual Model Participation in Recreation Opportunity Figure 1 indicates that one’s socioeconomic status (age, income/cost, education)‘ affects one’s opportunity (distance, ease of access) for recreation, which increases the likelihood of participating in a recreation setting. Socioeconomic status also directly affects recreation participation. There is no mention of race/ethnicity as having an effect. The study looked at low versus high income respondents, and users versus nonusers. Lindsay and Ogle’s approach to opportunity theory was unlike Hendee’s (1969) conception of opportunity theory (cited earlier). Lindsay and Ogle said that “participation rates in outdoor recreation vary directly with both cost and physical availability of outdoor recreation" (1972, p. 20). This definitional approach varies greatly from Hendee’s definition. For example, segregation (discrimination) issues were not considered, nor were issues related to being When explainlno figures In text. constructs wlll appear In Italics followed by the variable(s) In parentheses which reflect the construct. ' 28 ignorant of opportunities for recreation. In addition, Lindsay and Ogle’s sample was mostly white. In Craig’s (1972) study, his respondents were all southern Blacks. The reason for Craig’s inclusion in this selective review is that he offers insights that many researchers have forgotten (or chosen to ignore). Craig (1972) noted the following: white people, by their very efforts “to keep Negroes in their place" have done their utmost to prevent the emancipation of their leisure (p. 108) the effects of past experiences on immediate recreational behavior of Negroes may be greater than the effects of an increase in income or change in location (p. 111). Historically, recreational experiences of Negroes living in the Deep South have been extremely meager and limited. The important restraining variables were rigid segregation policies, low income, and lack of leisure time (p. 114). Aside from the fact that Craig’s arguments support the view that Blacks and Whites do not have similar leisure patterns, he argued for the ethnicity perspective, and pointed out that racial identification (not simply race) is an important variable to consider. In Figure 2, culture (White attitudes, plantation system) affects attitudes and behaviors associated with racial identity (racial identification, feelings of segregation). Racial identity has an affect on rural-urban residency and income. These constructs have an affect on recreation participation. Figure 2 reflects Craig's focus on suburban-urban Black differences. Craig made the point that Blacks define themselves in terms of White’s attitudes towards them. The reader has to put this study in context, however. The study took place in the South, in the early 1970s, and Blacks were ”made” urban once the 29 Figure 2. Craig’s (1972) Conceptual Model Rural/Urban Residency Attitudes Towards acial lde/ntily/ Recreation Participation Income area they lived in ”became" urban. In other words, there was no choice about living in an urban area. Craig showed the change from what was a rural area to what became an urban area. That is hard to find today. Rather than looking at how urban and suburban residency affects ethnic behavior, one should consider how ethnic behavior differs between urbanites and suburbanites. In today’s world, a comparable study would look at what happened to Blacks in urban areas when those areas were "made" inner-city areas. The pattern of rural to urban to inner-city residential stratification is reflective of eariier comments regarding residential segregation. Using secondary data, Washbume (1978) used a sub-sample technique that matched Blacks and Whites for the same characteristics. Results, however, should not have been generalized to the larger population. Figure 3 illustrates the conceptual model from Washbume's study. 30 Figure 3. Washbume’s (1978) Conceptual Model .——'—“ / \ \ r/ \\ \ w Perceived / \ \ \ Segregation / / \ \ . . __ ,, L 7 .. Participation Marginality in Recreati- In Figure 3, marginality (age, income, education, sex, residency, access, opportunities, poor transportation) affects participation (based on participation rates). Marginality was compared between Blacks and Whites. The portion of the model surrounded by the dotted-line circle was mentioned by Washbume, but never empirically measured in his study. This is Washbume’s greatest contribution to the scholarship on ethnic recreation studies, and his greatest shortcoming in his study. As a result, variables regarding preferences, social organization, and values were mentioned, but not examined. Klobus-Edwards (1981) provides one of the most elaborate conceptual models to data. Figure 4 is extremely complex. She considered the major factor to be race, residency (location in racially mixed neighborhood or in all Black neighborhood), sex, and marginality (age, education, household income, number in household). Motivation for recreation participation (physical conditioning, competition, relaxation, personal contacts, enjoyment, family time 31 [not looked at]) and barriers (lack of interesting programs, inadequate information, transportation problems, amount of time available for leisure activities) were intervening factors. Figure 4. Klobus-Edwards’ (1981) Conceptual Model Ma inal' Race In W Motivation . Leisure Participation Leisure participation was a composite dimension made up of recreation behavior (outdoor recreation, use of public recreation facilities, member in recreation associations) and recreation preference (willingness to participate in public sponsored activities). Recreation preference was used as a measure of leisure aspirations. An underlying assumpfion by Klobus-Edwards was that resources are located nearer to White areas, therefore Blacks living in racially mixed neighborhoods will participate fuller in recreation activities. The concepts of residency and motivation are introduced in a different manner in this model. Motivation reflects intrinsic reasons for wanting to 32 participate in leisure activities. Klobus-Edwards controlled for marginality variables in her study, and found that race explained between 9 and 26% of the variance in recreation involvement. She argues, but never tests, for a relationship between ethnicity and marginality perspectives (note that an arrow is missing between race and marginality). Accessibility to the recreation site (distance from place of residency) was not used as a variable. Transportation was used as a variable, but only as an inhibiting factor. The indicas created to reflect leisure participation behavior and preference were activity-related indicas, not behavior- related. Knowing what one participates in does not necessarily tell one how one feels toward an activity or towards leisure in general. Stamps and Stamps” (1985) study examined the relationships of race and class to leisure activities of urban residents. In their study, there were two major assumptions. The first was elaborated upon earlier: social integration occurs with residential integration. Their second assumption was that social class status determines ones life style (leisure participation was used as a proxy for life style). Figure 5 illustrates Stamps and Stamps’ conception of the interaction between the different constructs. As shown, the arrow from social class to leisure participation reflects their second assumption. Social class (education/occupation) status was determined by an index of social positioning (Hollingshead Two Factor Index) which created a five class index from education and occupation variables, and which Stamps and Stamps later collapsed into upper and lower class. 33 Figure 5. Stamps & Stamps” (1985) Conceptual Model Social Class \ Leisure Participation Ethnicity] - Race Figure 5 is reflective of the traditional paradigm established by Washbume in 1978. Stamps and Stamps concluded that, as explanatory variables, social class and race were still inconclusive as predictors of leisure activities. However, they found (contrary to what they expected) support for race, over class, as the more important predictor of leisure activities. Although they tried to approach social class in a new manner, ranking someone’s choice of leisure activities is not thly reflective of their lifestyle. lnfonnation came from a quality of life survey. A shortcoming in their study was that it was not a survey measuring attitudes or behaviors (as they relate to ethnic/racial or socioeconomic status) specifically, but rather they inferred from participation rates and preference based on rankings. As such, Stamps and Stamps recommended that future studies use more sophisticated multi-variate analysis of specific leisure activities. In addition, they recommended that leisure researchers abandon attempts to predict leisure participation from demographic variables. This further reinforces the need for more advanced models of analysis, as well as, reconceptualization of the unidimensional race construct. In contrast to Stamps and Stamps, and previous studies, Hutchison (1987) argues that a group’s social composition is more important for understanding differences and similarities between Blacks and Whites than looking at the specific activity they participated in. Figure 6 illustrates Hutchison’s conceptual model. In Figure 6, Hutchison looked at the influence that one’s ethnicity/race (Black, White, Hispanic) has on values (individual Versus family), social composition (peer groups, family groups, individual, sex composition, age composition, size of group), and leisure activities (mobile, stationary, sports activities). Hutchison’s study was different in some aspects. For example, he utilized a field observation approach instead of a questionnaire. The unit of analysis were not actual activities participated in, nor rates of participation, but rather the nature of the activity. Figure 6. Hutchison”s (1987) Conceptual Model Ethnicity! ..... Race Values Leisure Activities Social Composition Hutchison suggested “that black-white differences, if they exist, are due not to the simple influence of either class or race, but to a more complex (and not well understood) interaction between these two factors” (1987, p. 208). However, Hutchison neither tests this relationship, nor does he consider the role 35 of discrimination. The purpose of his study was to illustrate the advantage of observational techniques in supplementing quantitative data. Although Figure 6 illustrates observational techniques, there are some concerns regarding this technique. Hutchison, using Hispanic groups in his study, mentioned that distinctions must be made between different Hispanic groups (Mexicans were mostly looked at in his study). In addition, in observational data, it is much more difficult to distinguish dark-skinned Hispanics form Blacks. Therefore, group activity could seem to be intra-ethnic or intra- racial when, in fact, it is Hispanic. Also, generational issues could be further explored. If one assumes that the dominant culture exudes influence, then second and third generation Hispanics should be closely aligned (in their recreation behavior) to Whites, than their immigrant and first generation counterparts. West (1989) noted that discrimination affects marginality and subcultures (racial/ethnic groups). West’s study took place in Detroit and data was collected via phone interviews. A critical contribution by West was that he operationalized the concepts of marginality (limitations on participation due to limited income or limited access to transportation), subculture (difference in unconstrained preference by minority groups, i.e., "no interest” and "prefer to do other things"), and interracial factors (operationalized from open-ended questions and collapsed into five categories: racism, racial-anti-white, racial-anti-black, race relations, and uncomfortable/u nwelcomed) (West, 1989, pp. 16-1 7). 36 Figure 7 illustrates how West incorporated discrimination and preference into the traditional paradigm (see Figure 5). Race (Blacks and Whites) and marginality (subjective and objective measures of income, transportation) have an effect on both discrimination (interracial factors (perceived or real), and use of parks (use/non-use, frequency of use). Race also affects subcultural preference (no interest, prefer to do other things), which in turn affects use of urban parks. West, however, called for a more robust measure of subcultural preference. Figure 7. West’s (1989) Conceptual Model Subcultural -- Preference \ Urban Use Marginality West (1989) found that Whites” use of the tri-county parks and frequency of use of tri-county parks in Detroit was significantly greater than Blacks’ use of tri-county parks. In addition, he found that marginality played a small role in minority underrepresentation, and found no support for Black subculture as a reason for underrepresentation in Detroit’s tri-county parks. This opened the door for alternative explanations. West found that transportation was perceived as a major barrier. It was also surmised that interracial relations may be a contributing factor in inhibiting use of regional parks by inner—city minorities in 37 Detroit. Furthermore, West raised the issue of perceived geographic distance (subjective and objective measures) as a possible factor influencing urban park usage (West, 1989, pp. 18-25). West’s contribution to minority recreation studies is invaluable because he underscores three major initiatives. First, although he found no support for subcultural explanations, he maintains that Blacks are still under represented because of other factors that have not been measured, or have not been measured adequately. Such factors could be physical-structural (location), political (policies regarding or restricting use), economical (lower discretionary income), or cultural-structural (education system, political system, residential patterns and housing issues, economic base for infrastructure improvements in inner-city). The point is that marginality and ethnicity are not explanatory enough, and they do not operate in a vacuum. Second, West noted that the policy problem is not one of "access to recreation areas by minorities, but rather the quality of recreation facilities and services” (1989, p. 25). Policies regarding urban parks need to focus on bringing the parks to the users (inner-city Blacks in this case), not bringing the users to the parks. One need only take a cursory look at conditions in inner-city neighborhoods to see that there have been disinvestments in inner-city parks (in part due to crime). Perhaps new initiatives involving coalitions with grass-roots organizations and police foot and bike patrols may be in order. West’s third contribution was a challenge to leisure researchers "to face more directly the issues of race relations"(1989, p. 25). It took a decade after 38 Washbume’s study to incorporate the issue of discrimination into minority recreation studies; yet another reason why the recreation researchers (investigating minority issues) should be familiar with the literature on race relations. Taylor (1992,1993) advanced the current understanding of the ethnic recreation literature (Figure 8). Her study was based on personal interviews of the people and urban parks in New Haven, Connecticut in 1989. She considered several recommendations of previous recreation researchers. Taylor considered Woodard”s assertion that “the problem with the ethnicity model is that is assumes that ethnic groups have a single value and normative order, [thus], there can be no variation in leisure among [said groups]'” (1988, p. 88). Figure 8. A Model of the Factors Affecting Ethnic Leisure Participation Social -_.._,-_--fl-.,,.. _. LL ___> Class ‘* , ’ Race—t— , ‘L ..... L: Participate In Ethnic gig—mm] j‘i _ I m “i-Tmem ’ Life Cycle , A ,7. “' ' '[Sex in Marital Status ' . 'I { Community Age -_.,, ,,,-_,-,. ‘HTTTT ‘* I_r____ __,.,..L,,,-_m ,v,,* is, 4 Source: Taylor, D. E. (1992). Identity in ethnic leisure pursuits. San Francisco, CA: Mellon Research University Press, p. 36. 39 She accomplished this by looking at both racial and ethnic groups (showing clear support for distinguishing between the two concepts). The racial groups consisted of Blacks and Whites. Ethnic groups were Jamaican and African Americans for Blacks, and Italians and "other whites" for Whites. This allowed for a comparison between and within groups. Taylor (1992) created a more complex model. She called it the "differential access model” (see Figure 8). As Figure 8 indicates, ethnic leisure participation is a very complex phenomenon. The impact of this model is greater than it seems. In prior studies, differences, which were not explained by marginality variables, were said to be explained by ethnicity variables, and vice- versa. Taylor's model illustrates what later researchers have been asserting: the factors influencing ethnic leisure participation need to go beyond marginality and ethnicity theories, and consider other factors. Taylor (1992) addressed the shortcomings of her book. She mentioned that her next project was to accomplish two things: ”(1) refine and test the model developed in [her] project, and (2) attempt a more sophisticated analysis of the questions raised in [her] book" (Taylor, 1992, Preface). Indeed, the model is rather cumbersome. However, it is a start. In her model, Taylor did not address the role of racism and discrimination. In addition, her variables could have been subjected to a factor analytic approach to verify the existence of her constructs. With respect to her eartier consideration of subcultures, Taylor noted the following: The results of this study do not support a subcultural thesis because there were important and striking differences between 40 African Americans and Jamaicans, the two Black ethnic groups being studied. It the Black leisure subculture theses were true, such striking within-group (Black) differences would not be found (1992,p.259) In the traditional view of the Black leisure subcultural thesis, the thesis assumes all Blacks have a normative homogeneous subculture. However, this thesis considered African Americans and Whites, not ethnic groups who happened to be considered Black. The thesis assumes that African American Blacks are a subculture of Whites. Because of their phenotype, White Americans consider Jamaicans Black. It does not mean, however, that they share the same values, experiences, and history with African American Blacks. The fact that there were differences between Jamaican Blacks and African American Blacks proves subculture (a subculture within a subculture). Taylor makes the same mistake other researchers do when lumping Cubans, Puerto Ricans, Mexicans, and Panamanians under one category: Hispanic. One has to keep in mind that racial terms such as Black and White, and ethnic terms such as Caucasian, Asian, or Hispanic are socially constmcted terms for categorical purposes. Taylor was looking at two distinct cultural groups. Perhaps a better method of disproving the subcultural hypothesis is to look at those who identify themselves as African American Blacks. Perhaps the subcultural thesis needs to be refined or renamed. Taylor’s use of subculture more appropriately considers sub-racial , not cultural differences. If differences between Blacks and Whites don’t hold true, then the sub-racial thesis would also not be supported. 41 In response to the restriction of scope of the marginality-ethnicity paradigm, Floyd, et al. (1993) incorporated the marginality-ethnicity theory within a broader context: assimilation theory. "The assimilation perspective is broad enough to encompass the marginality-ethnicity paradigm and can also accommodate a perceived discrimination dimension” (Floyd, et al., 1993, p. 84). This was an attempt to integrate concepts from Washbume (1978) and West (1989), and is reflected in Figure 9. Figure 9. Conceptual Model of Explanations of Ethnic Variation in Recreational Behavior A c an . Cultural Subcultural CUE omen” @ \ Structural ___ Socioeconomic Marginality _., Behavioral Intergroup Receptional W- . h- @ Source: Floyd, M.F., Gramann, J.H., 8 Saenz, R. (1993). Ethnic factors and the use of public outdoor recreation areas: The case of Mexican Americans. W15 (2), p. 86. RECREATION BEHAVIOR Floyd, et al., (1993) also noted that past approaches involving ethnicity and recreational behavior focused on Black and White individuals. Their study used Mexican-Americans as their research population. The data collection technique used was telephone interviews. There were three major concepts which came from this study that contributed to the growing literature regarding ethnicity and recreation behavior. 42 The three dimensions were three of seven subprocesses identified by Gordon in 1964: acculturation, stnrctural assimilation, and behavioral receptional assimilation. These three dimensions respectively correspond to the three types of social distance: cultural distance, socioeconomic distance, and intergroup distance (Floyd, et al., 1993, p. 85). In the current study’s use of acculturation, although minorities are ”acquiring“ the dominant group’s cultural characteristics, they continue to maintain their own cultural characteristics as well. The dominant culture exerts pressure on the minority culture to move from acculturation to full assimilation, but the process of assimilation is tempered by the minority’s level of exposure to the dominant culture. Acculturation is therefore closely related to Floyd, et al.’s concept of ”cultural distance” (M)- Cultural distance,(acculturation) was measured by a respondent’s language preference for home, television, radio, and literature. Socioeconomic distance was measured by level of education. Intergroup distance (perceived discrimination) was measured by asking questions about feelings of acceptance at the recreation site, other ethnic groups at the recreation site, and hassle from police at the recreation site. Floyd, at al., acknowledged procedural shortcomings. Figure 9 illustrates Floyd, et al.”s conception of the factors affecting ethnic group participation in recreation activities. The perceived discrimination factor was found to be insignificant. Floyd, et al., recommended that future research develop measures of discrimination that correspond closely to visitation at specific areas. Although 43 their study could have had stronger measures/statistical design to incorporate relative causality by use of the coefficients, their study is an important contribution because it reoonceptualized the marginality-ethnicity paradigm within a larger perspective: assimilation theory. Chapter 3 METHODOLOGY Movin Forw : Th D v I m nt of a Model Given the aforementioned conceptual review, the current researcher seeks to create a model which identifies major concepts, and considers how these concepts are (inter)related. Figure 10 illustrates the relationships between the following factors: Acculturation (ACC), Socioeconomic status (SES), Subcultural identity (SID), Benefits of Recreation (BEN), Perceived Discrimination (DIS), and Recreation Participation (REC). Figure 10. The Ethnicity and Public Recreation Participation Model “a Perceived Benefits of _ Recreation , Recmatron H Participation Perceived Discrimination Subcultural Identity The Ethnicity and Public Recreation Participation Model (EPRPM) illustrates several of the components reviewed in the literature. The EPRPM is a causal model. It reflects the literature’s support of these concepts and how each concept relates to others. It is important to understand the order of causation. 45 Davis (1985) supplies some rules concerning the logic of causal order (for two variables X and Y, distinguish among three possibilities): (1) X—>Y, X might influence Y but Y does not influence X. (2) X F’ Y, X and Y might influence each other. (3) XHY, X and Y might correlate highly, but we do not know directionality (p. 10). There are three important things to remember. First, the directions of the arrows in a causal model indicate "potential” flows of causation, not necessarily actual flows. Second, the most important principle of causal order is that ”after cannot cause before there is no way to change the past one way arrows flow with time" (Davis, 1985, p. 11). The last thing to remember about causality is the following: most methodologists agree that causal order is a substantive or empirical problem to be solved by our knowledge about how the real world works, not by statistical gyrations. At the philosophical level this has positive and negative implications. On the negative side, empirical research is always hostage to empirical assumptions that might be wrong; on the positive side, computers cannot substitute for sociologists in analyzing data, because computers do not know anything about the real worid and sociologists do know a little bit (Davis, 1985, p. 11). Given this insight into the development of causal relationships, one can now consider Figure 10 in its entirety. Acculturation is believed to measure one”s level of assimilation into the dominant culture. The causal arrows extend to both socioeconomic status and subcultural identification. The underlying assumption is that acculturation begins when one is born and the level of assimilation changes over time. Acculturation differs slightly from Floyd, et al.’s (1993) conception. Rather than occurring simultaneously with socioeconomic variables (see Figure 9) and 46 subcultural identification, it is conceptualized, in the current study, as a precursor to SES and SID. The stronger one’s level of acculturation (cultural distance) the stronger one will identify with a majority culture. Conversely, if one”s level of acculturation is weak, then the socioeconomic status will be lower. For example, if one speaks fluent English, the prospect of a better paying job will be increased. This causal relationship reflects acculturation theory. Following the arrows, one can see a direct relationship between socioeconomic status and recreation participation. This relationship is hypothesized by the literature. It reflects the marginality theory. The arrow from subcultural identity to recreation behavior reflects the ethnicity theory. Both socioeconomic status and subcultural identity have two intervening constructs which are perceived discrimination and perceived recreation benefits. BEN is used as an intervening variable because it was thought that the perceived benefits of a recreation activity or recreation site will affect actual participation in the activity or at the recreation site. The ”perception of recreation benefits” construct incorporates KIobus-Edwards” (1981) suggestion of integrating ”motivation for recreation participation” as a precursor to actual participation. The assumption is that motivations or perceived benefits derived from participation in a recreation activity (or going to a recreation site) is based on one”s socioeconomic status and cultural perspective. For example, there are two local parks, and one has a football game and the other has a baseball game. If you ask a Puerto Rican which he/she would 47 prefer to go to, the odds are that helshe would pick the baseball game over the football game because football is not a national sport in Puerto Rico, and baseball is a national sport. Following the causal model, the motivation or benefit (watching baseball) of going to the park (passive recreation) is influenced by one’s culture (Puerto Rican). Conversely, if one had the economic means to attend a professional baseball game (instead of the local baseball game), or considers the attendance at a professional baseball game to give one certain social status, than socioeconomic status would affect the perception of the benefits of visiting a local baseball park. In addition, the model postulates that socioeconomic status will affect one’s perception of discrimination. The assumption here is that as one increases in socioeconomic standing, helshe may perceive less discrimination because of opportunities afforded by one”s socioeconomic status. As one’s socioeconomic status increases, perceived discrimination decreases. A decrease in perceived discrimination causes an increase in recreation participation. Perceived discrimination reflects the use of West’s (1989) discrimination perspective. It is affected by one’s subcultural identity. The stronger one considers him/herself a member of a subcultural group, helshe will have a stronger perception of discrimination. This then affects recreation behavior because the perception of discrimination may act as a deterrent to participation in recreation activities or sites. 48 Table 2 summarizes the direct and indirect influences of the different concepts on recreation behavior. The only affect not shown in Table 2 is the relationship between SES and SID. In the literature, there is no support as to which construct influences the other. There is support for showing that there is a relationship, and that this relationship is spurious. The model illustrates that SES and SID have a common antecedent: ACC, which drives both of them, thereby making SES and SID spuriously related by definition. Table 2 Direct and Indirect Effects of Ethnicmr' Constructs onLecrestion Participation Constructs Direct Effects Indirect Effects ACC ACC—iSES ACC—rSES—iREC ACC—vSID ACC—vSES—+BEN—i REC ACC—+SES—iDIS—+REC ACC—+SlD—iDlS—+REC ACC—iSID—vBEN-rREC ACC—+SID—9REC SES SES—iREC SES—iBEN—bREC SES-iDIS—iREC SID SlD—iREC SID-+BEN—iREC SID—iDIS—iREC BEN BEN—iREC None DIS DIS—*REC None Conceptually, one can see that there are both direct and indirect effects that influence recreation behavior. The EPRPM exemplifies a multiple causation model. The indirect effects and direct effects combine to provide a more 49 comprehensive picture of the relationships and interrelationships of the different concepts mentioned in the literature regarding ethnicity and recreation behavior. The EPRPM reconceptualizes the previous research by offering an alternative framework. This framework is much more involved than the traditional marginality-ethnicity framework and incorporates other factors identified as critical in the assessment of ethnic groups and recreation behavior. Qgefiionnairs Dssign Each of the constructs identified in the EPRP Model requires a scale for measurement of the particular construct. The scales are made up of three to eight items per construct. The constructs are as follows and each will be considered in turn: (1) Acculturation, (2) Subcultural Identity, (3) Perceived Discrimination, (4) Perceived Benefits of Recreation, (5) Socioeconomic Status, (6) Recreation Participation (park use). Assglturation. Acculturation was defined by Floyd, et al. (1993) as ”cultural distance.” They measured acculturation by asking respondents to report their preferred language for use at home, radio listening and television viewing, and reading newspapers and magazines. Their response categories were: (1) English only; (2) mostly English, some Spanish; (3) both English and Spanish equally; (4) mostly Spanish, some English; (5) mostly Spanish. The fifth choice should have read ”Spanish only" for consistency. Barona 8 Miller designed a short acculturation scale to ”sample cultural behaviors from both the context of the family and the context of extrafamilial social and media influences" (1994, p. 156). Their scale was designed for 50 Hispanic youth. They noted that previous acculturation scales used sociodemographic data, and recommended that sociodemographic items be excluded from acculturation scales. The use of the acculturation scale by Floyd, et al. (1993) and Barona 8 Miller (1993) is consistent with traditional uses of acculturation scales. In a study on first and second generation Puerto Ricans, Cortes, Rogler 8 Malgady argue that there is an overemphasis in acculturation scales on language usage," and that the level of involvement in Puerto Rican or American2 culture as well as other elements of culture should also be examined (1994, p. 711). The point is well taken. The process of acculturation goes beyond language and incorporates values, norms, and beliefs. Cortes, at al. (1994) consider the extent to which one is bicultural as an important indicator as well. They developed a 20 item scale (10 items to reflect Puerto Rican culture and 10 items to reflect American culture) from a focus group of Puerto Ricans in New York City. The themes reflected in the items include "language preference and usage, values, ethnic pride, food preferences, child-bearing practices, and interpersonal relations” (Cortes, et al., 1994, p. 712). Saldafia (1994) remarked that acculturation scales often incorporate an individual”s ethnicity and his/her ethnic identity. Ethnicity, in this case, was defined as belonging to a particular ethnic group. Ethnic identity "may more accurately reflect internal factors associated with level of acculturation” (p. 117). 2 In ordertodlstlngulshtrom PuerloRIcans, 'Amerimns' will be used because It alludesto ctrlmraldlfferencesbasedonnatlonallsflcllnes. PuertoRIcansraciallybelongtobothWhlte, BlackandNatlveAmericanraces,butethnicallybelongtonelther. 51 The Saldafla study reflects the current study”s operational use of subcultural identity (see next section). Saldafia believed that acculturation and ethnic identity should be two constructs. Of particular relevance to the current study is Saldafia’s ”behavioral preference" sub-scale as a measure of acculturation. This sub-scale (factor) accounted for 52% of the variance and used items that were similar to the Floyd, et al., and Cortes, at al., studies. Items from these previous studies were used to measure acculturation in the current study by considering items that loaded highest, items which were similar across studies, and alpha levels of .75 or higher. In addition, a pilot study of 20 Puerto Ricans helped to discern the most relevant items. Table 3 shows the items and the articles they were taken from.3 In Table 3, the responses for items 1-7 are based on a five-point scale measuring (1) only Spanish, (2) more Spanish than English, (3) both equally, (4) more English than Spanish, and (5) only English. The responses for items 8-11 are also based on a five-point scale: (1) very important, (2) important, (3) somewhat important, (4) a little important, and (5) unimportant. Subcultural identig. Saldafia’s (1994) ”ethnic loyalty" sub-scale reflects ona”s ethnic identity. She considered ethnic identification as complimentary to, but different from, acculturation. Figure 10, reviewed previously, reflects this conceptual relationship. Four items made up the scale (alpha = .81 ): identification with culture, bilingualism, cultural pride, and participation in cultural actiVIties. 3 Theltemsllstedlnthetablesretlectthelnltlalwordinganduseoftheltemstorthepretests. ThereadershouldrefertothequestionnalrelnAppendleforthefinalwordlngoftheltems. 52 Table 3 The Aflltupstipn Scale Item Source 1. Preferred language at home Floyd, at al. (1993) 2. Language preference for radio Floyd, et al. (1993), Saldaiia and television. (1994) 3. Language preference for Floyd, et al. (1993), Saldaila reading magazines, and (1994) newspapers. 4. Language preference for Saldafta (1994) music. 5. What language do your close Barona 8 Miller (1994) friends speak? 6. You prefer going to parties Barona 8 Miller (1994) which speak which language? 7. Which language do the people Barona 8 Miller (1994) you visit or that visit you speak? 8. How important is it to celebrate Cortes, Rogler and Malgady holidays in the American way? (1994) 9. How important is it to celebrate Cortes, Rogler and Malgady holidays in the Puerto Rican way? (1994) 10. How much are Puerto Rican Cortes, Rogler and Malgady values a part of your life? (1994) 11. How much are American Cortes, Rogler and Malgady values a part of your life? (1994) She correctly noted that "[little] research has addressed the relationship of ethnic identity to level of acculturation, and these two constructs are often treated synonymously'” (Saldafla, 1994, p. 117). Because of the synonymous 53 nature involved in ethnic studies, most scales measuring identity have been developed for racial identity (see Burlew 8 Smith, 1991, for an excellent review). Scales specifically measuring ethnocentrism have been lacking. Reasons for this range from operationalization of ethnicity and race to transitional definitions between race and ethnicity. ”For member of racial minorities, social categories of race or national origin override the importance of ethnicity. Their “choice” is limited because their identity is constrained to labels imposed by other members of the society or by custom” (Xie 8 Goyette, 1997, pp. 549-550). Rodriguez accentuates this particular point with the Puerto Ricans in New York City. The experience of Puerto Ricans in New York City points up more clearly than any researched materials the chasm that exists between whites and blacks in the United States and the racism that afflicts both groups. For within the US. perspective, Puerto Ricans, racially speaking belong to both groups; however, ethnically, they belong to neither (1996, p. 25). Few scales have been developed specifically for, or tested among, Puerto Ricans. Because of this inattention, the scale measuring Puerto Rican cultural identity was adopted from the African Self-Conscious (ASC) scale (Baldwin 8 Bell, 1985). The scale consists of 42 items measuring beliefs, attitudes, and opinions of Black people. It is an overall measure of afrocentricity with reliability of .90. There are both individual and group measures of subcultural identity. A later analysis of the ASC scale suggested only 32 of the 42 items be retained (Stokes, Murray, Peacock, 8 Kaiser, 1994). Whether 42 or 32 scale items, it is an overwhelming number of items for the current study. The researcher limited the items to seven (six of which came from the ASC scale). 54 Items were selected according to how the wording reflected the subcultural identity construct. Item 4 was reworded to reflect a stronger Individual focus. Table 4 illustrates the original items and how they were re-worded to reflect the current study population. Table 4 Sub ltu I I nt' cal Original Wording of ASC Scale Ra-Wording 1. Blacks born in the United States are 1. I strongly identify myself as a Puerto Black or African First, rather than Rican first, rather than as an American. American or just plain people. 2. Blacks in America should try harder to 2. Puerto Ricans in America should try be American rather than practicing harder to be American rather than activities that link them up with their practicing activities that connect them to African cultural heritage their cultural heritage. 3. Regardless of their Interests, 3. Regardless of their interests, educational background, and social educational background, and social achievements, I would prefer to associate achievements, I would prefer to associate with black people than with non-blacks. with Puerto Ricans. 4. Black children should be taught that 4. When I was growing up, my parents they are African people at an early age. taught me that my Puerto Rican culture is very distinct, different from the American culture. 5. I feel little sense of commitment to 5. I feel little sense of commitment to Black people who are not close friends or other Puerto Ricans who are not close relatives. friends or relatives. 6. White people, generally speaking, do 6. Americans, generally speaking, do not not respect Black life. respect the Puerto Rican culture. 7. Not in ASC Scale. 7. If I had a choice, I would rather hold the Puerto Rican flag, than the American flag. As one can see from Table 4, in addition to the six items from the ASC scale, a seventh item was added to reflect a characteristic particular to the Puerto Rican cultural identity: nationalism. Items 4 and 7 reflect this dimension. 55 Unlike Blacks in the US, national and cultural symbols (e.g., Puerto Rican flag, el coqui, aljibaro) are defining characteristics of the Puerto Rican, not racial identity. Perhaps the primary point of contrast is that in Puerto Rico, racial identity is subordinate to cultural identity, while in the US, racial identification, to a large extent, determines cultural identification. Thus when asked that divisive question, "What are you?” Puerto Ricans of all colors and ancestry answer, "Puerto Rican,” while most New Yorkers answer black, Jewish, or perhaps ”'of Italian descent'” (Rodriguez, 1996, p. 26). Perceived gissrimination. Several researchers have looked at the issue of perceived discrimination and comfort level with respect to recreation (Woodard, 1988; West 1989; Chavez, 1993; and Falk, 1995). Feagin (1991) looked at antiblack discrimination in public places and focused on (1) the sites of discrimination, (2) the character of discrimination actions, and (3) the range of coping responses by Blacks to discrimination (p. 101). Feagin listed discriminatory action against Blacks as five progressively serious actions: (1) avoidance actions, such as white couple crossing the street when a black male approaches; (2) reject actions, such as poor service in public accommodations; (3) verbal attacks, such as shouting racial epithets in the street; (4) physical threats and harassment by white police officers; and (5) physical threats and attacks by other whites, such as attacks by white supremacists, in the street (1991, p. 102). Given these factors, items were developed by borrowing from two studies. Two items (Items 1 and 2 in Table 5) came from Floyd, et al.”s (1993) study. The remaining three came from Sénchez 8 Brook’s (1996) study (T able 5). Item 2 was mentioned by several researchers as a factor in public areas (Feagin, 1991; Chavez, 1993; Floyd, et al., 1993). Sénchez 8 Brock looked at the relationship 56 between perceived discrimination among Hispanics in the work setting. Their hypotheses suggested a positive relationship between subcultural identity and perception of discrimination. The items from the Sénchez 8 Brock scale were reworded to reflect public parks instead of a work setting. Table 5 Perceiv i ' in ti n. Item Source 1. In some Southbridge parks, Floyd, Gramman, 8 Saenz (1993) Puerto Ricans are not very welcomed. 2. In Southbridge parks, police Floyd, Gramman, 8 Saenz (1993) often hassle Puerto Ricans. 3. In Southbridge parks, the Sénchez 8 Brock (1996) presence of other groups that are not Puerto Rican makes me feel uncomfortable. 4. In Southbridge parks, I feel that Sénchez 8 Brock (1996) Americans exclude me from activities because I am Puerto Rican. 5. In Southbridge parks, Sénchez 8 Brock (1996) Americans look down on me because I participate in customs of my culture. Psrgivfl mnefits pf [sorgation There are a number of studies and scales which have been developed to assess the cognitive nature of leisure and recreation (lso—Ahola, 1980; Neulinger, 1981; Witt 8 Ellis, 1989). The items in 57 the Perceived Benefits scale (items 1A-1G, see Appendix A) reflect key concepts in the recreation literature (e.g., relaxation, escape, socialization, family time, and physical fitness). The items are based on lso-Ahola”s Benefits of Leisure Scale. The wording was altered from ”benefit of leisure" to ”benefit of going to Southbridge parks.” The idea was to make the term ”leisure” more specific to the leisure activity under study: park use. W. The most commonly used scale for socioeconomic status is Hollingshead”s Two-Factor Index of Social Positioning (1965), or his more current version: the Four-Factor Index of Social Status (1975). Edwards-Hewitt and Gray (1995) provide an excellent review of other socioeconomic scales, as well as a comparison between objective and subjective reporting formats on these scales by ethnic group members. For this study, the researcher contacted Gray to obtain the latest version of Hollingshead”s index. However, the index was obtained after the current study was undenrvay. As a result, the researcher altered the original index and adapted it to reflect the socioeconomic questions asked in the current study. Hollingshead’s Four-Factor Social Status scale used the items of marital status, education, occupation, and gender. Because the demographics section of the current study’s questionnaire (see Appendix A) did not reflect the Hollingshead categories (e.g., the questionnaire did not ask for both the husband and the wife’s information to combine for a particular status score), it was decided to use Hollingshead”s original Two-Factor Index which included the education and occupation as 58 items. Edwards-Hewitt and Gray also noted (from Gordon,1958) that some studies use occupation in their socioeconomic scales, while others use income. The current study does not use income because income was reported on a household basis, whereas occupation and education are based on an individual basis. In addition, the researcher felt that gender and marital status (used in the Four-Factor Index) are inappropriate variables to include in a scale of socioeconomic status because of the nominal nature of the items. In the actual Four-Factor Hollingshead scale, the gender and marital status are incorporated into the education and occupation measures. Therefore, one still has a two factor scale. Additionally, gender and marital status could have an impact on other parts of the model (e.g., they could be intervening or mediating variables), and to include them in only one construct does not seem meaningful for this study. The socioeconomic scale used for this study uses the items education, occupation, and income. Education was scored from 1 to 7. In the questionnaire, there are nine categories, but the last three were combined into one category (graduate school) in order to match Hollingshead’s seven-point scale. Occupation was scored from 0 to 9 based on Hollingshead’s 1975 designation for occupation. Income was scored from 1 to 9. The categories for income reflect the US. Census breakdown (see Appendix A). W. Park use is the final endogenous construct in the EPRP Model. There are five public parks in the study area. Recreation 59 participation was measured by asking the annual frequency of use (by season) for each of the parks (see Appendix A). The five items measuring park use are the total visits to each park by a respondent. The seven scores for park use by season were: (1) never (scored 0); (2) once (scored 1); (3) almost never/ 2-4 times (scored 3); (4) sometimes! 5-13 times (scored 9); (5) frequent! 14-20 times (scored 17); (6) very frequent! 21-29 times (scored 25); (7) a lot/ 30+ times (scored 35). Each score represents the middle value. For example, if a respondent checked the fourth box on the questionnaire (sometimes), helshe falls between 5 and 13, and would be given a score of ”'9.” The five items measuring parks were combined to give an overall assessment of participation in each of the city's public parks by respondent. For the purpose of this study, the interest is not in the actual activities engaged in the park, but whether or not the ethnic group members visited the public recreation sites (parks), and if so, how frequent. As a result, each respondent was given a "scale score” from 0 to 6 for use of each park. The constmction of the scale categories is reflected in Table 6. Using Table 6, if one were to go to West Street Park (see Appendix A) 14-20 times in the Summer, once in the Fall, never in the Winter and 5—13 times in the Spring, then the respondent would be assigned the following designation: 17+1+0+9 = 27. The total use for West Street Park is 27 and it falls within the “total range" of 13-36, therefore the respondent would be coded a "3"; a 60 "somewhat frequent“ user of West Street Park. This is done for every park. All five parks are then combined into an index to measure park use. Table 6 Scoring of Paik Use for Esch Park Ssason Annual Total Scale Annual Use Scars Maximum Rsnge Me Dssignation for Each Park 0 0 0 Never 1 4 1-4 1 Very Few 3 12 5-12 2 A Few Times 9 36 13-36 3 Somewhat Frequent 17 68 37—68 4 Frequent 25 100 69-100 5 Very Frequent 35 140 101-140 6 A Lot Sub'ects and m lin Frame In order to cany out this study, access to an ethnic group population was needed to empirically test the different theoretical constructs. The ethnic group members selected were Puerto Rican. This ethnic group has not been researched in previous ethnic recreation studies. Because of the relationship between the United States (US) and Puerto Rico, Puerto Ricans provide researchers with an opportunity to look at acculturation processes, perceived 61 discrimination, socioeconomic factors, issues regarding cultural identity, and a unique perspective on their perceptions of parks. Puerto Ricans are part of the growing Latino population. It was important to consider a Latino group for three reasons. First, from a demographic standpoint, Latinos will be the largest ethnic group at the beginning of the next millennium. Second, previous research regarding ethnicity and recreation has focused primarily on African Americans. Third, the researcher needed to have access to the ethnic group members. Because the researcher is Puerto Rican, and a native of the study area, the researcher had access to the population, and an understanding of the population and its cultural nuances to facilitate participation in the study. GQgpaphis profils. This study was conducted in Southbridge, Massachusetts. Southbridge is located in Worcester County - south-central Massachusetts (MA), and borders northern Connecticut. The city is hilly, particularly in the northern and southern part of the city. The central part of the city is in a valley. Southbridge is approximately 60 miles west of Boston, MA. All parks are located approximately within one mile of the downtown area (see Appendix B). W According to the 1990 Census, Southbridge”s population was 17,816. The median household income for Southbridge residents is $27, 834. Puerto Ricans constitute the largest ethnic group in Southbridge. Puerto Ricans represent 12% (2,109) of the city’s population, and 91% of the city's Hispanic population. Over half of the Puerto Rican population (56%) 62, was born in Puerto Rico. Spanish is spoken in nearly 10% of the entire population, and by about 75% of all Hispanics (United States Census, 1990). The first Puerto Rican family arrived in Southbridge in 1957 (Brown, 1982). Puerto Rico, at the time, was transitioning from an agricultural economy to an industrial economy. As a result, Puerto Rico’s agrarian labor force turned to the US for economic relief. Specifically, Southbridge’s Prest-Wheel Company hired many Puerto Ricans in the late 19503 and early 19603(Brown, 1982; Datz, 1998) The late 19503 marked the beginning of serious Puerto Rican migration into Central Massachusetts. For the first few years, the most frequent landing spot was an industrial town accustomed to employing immigrants: Southbridge. (Datz, 1998, p. A23). By the late 19603, Puerto Ricans were attracted to Southbridge because of its need for an unskilled labor force to work in industry. Those Puerto Ricans who arrived in the 19603 paved the way for the next wave in the 19703. The majority of those arriving spoke only Spanish. Strong family ties to Puerto Rico were maintained because families often left siblings and parents behind. Southbridge Puerto Ricans travel to Puerto Rico quite often and send money and clothing to their extended families in Puerto Rico. The majority of the Puerto Ricans arriving in Southbridge, if not related, already knew each other from their barrios, neighboring towns, or family friends. As a result, an enclave of Puerto Ricans was established in Southbridge which reinforced ethnic cohesion. Southbridge Puerto Ricans are particularly suited to this study because of the strong ties to their homeland. The respondents would be emigrant, first, or second generation Puerto Ricans in Southbridge. 63 This study comes at a time that marks Puerto Rico’s 100‘” anniversary under US rule. A plebiscite was recently conducted in December of 1998. The researcher considered what possible impact this might have in the nationalistic tendencies of some of the Puerto Ricans in Southbridge. The impact was considered to be minimal. The local and county newspapers (Southbridge Evening News, Worcester Telegram 8 Gazette) reported opinions of the plebiscite and the results of the plebiscite. In both instances, the papers reported the Puerto Rican communities (on both sides of the Carribean) as being divided about the issue of statehood for Puerto Rico (Datz, 1998; Echegaray, 1998; Rabinowitz, 1998; “Puerto Ricans,” 1998). As a result of the focus on Latinos in Worcester County, Massachusetts, the county paper reported that the majority of Latinos in the county are from Puerto Rico or of Puerto Rican descent, and that of the six communities in the county with the largest number of Latinos, Southbridge has the largest Latino population for any one city in Massachusetts:16.7% (2,999) of the Southbridge population as of 1995 (Echegaray, 1998). Population and sample size. The subjects were selected from the Puerto Rican population of Southbridge, MA. Subjects were 14 years of age or older. Because high schoolers are active users of Southbridge parks, it was important to include them in this study. High schoolers offer a broader age variance which may illustrate generational influences on participation at public recreation sites. In addition, because a majority of Puerto Ricans in Southbridge were born in Puerto Rico, it 64 is expected that their perspective on American values and their acculturation and assimilation will be different than their children’s and grandchildren’s perspectives. The sample size for this study was determined by three values: (1) alpha value (for Type I error), (2) power (for Type II error), and (3) effect size (how large a difference one wants to be able to detect in one”s study) (Shavelson, 1996). The level of significance for this study will be set at the .05 level. The probability of rejecting a false null hypothesis is set at .90 (1-8). The researcher would like to have statistical tests detect a standardized difference of .20 (effect size). Using Table M in Shavelson’s (1996) text, the approximate sample size for a one-tailed test design is N=215. For a two-tailed test design, N=262. The majority of the hypotheses in this study consider a one-tail design, however there is one hypothesis that requires a two-tailed test. Therefore, a minimum of 262 subjects is considered adequate for this study. This is consistent with Kline’s assertion that the larger the sample the better, and that samples of “less than 100 could produce misleading results” in factorial designs (1994, p.180). In addition to the statistical power needed, 262 subjects represent 19% of the Southbridge Hispanic population that is older than 14 years (1,397), a more than adequate sample size for inference to the population. 1 WWW For the selection of Puerto Rican high schoolers, the researcher met with the superintendent of schools and 65 the high school principal to obtain permission for the administration of the questionnaire during home room period. A copy of the questionnaire, the human subjects approval form from Michigan State University, and a letter of introduction were provided for them prior to administration approval. It was mentioned that participation was strictly voluntary and that all information is anonymous and confidential. Permission was verbally granted from both the superintendent and the principal. The high school liaison was the head of the Social Studies curriculum in the school system. Explanations and instructions were given to him on how the researcher wanted to conduct the study. Home room teachers were instructed on how to administer the questionnaire and to return the completed questionnaires to the Principal’s Office. A count of the number of Puerto Ricans in the High School was obtained and questionnaires were provided in both English and Spanish. According to a breakdown by home rooms, there were a total of 135 Latino high school students (W. Gosk 8 J. P. Bailey, personal communication, January, 1999). The high school liaison was instructed to deliver the box of surveys to the researcher’s sister (who works in the school system) (see Appendix B). She mailed the surveys back to the researcher. Prosfiures for adult population sample selection. In order to sample the Puerto Rican adult population, key persons in the Puerto Rican community had to be contacted. These community leaders have access to lists of names, or have contact with Puerto Ricans at Puerto Rican-owned establishments. This 66 process involved tapping into the Puerto Rican community’s social capital by utilizing formal and informal networks to obtain verbal consent and addresses of prospective respondents. Such contacts were made in the following community areas: - St. Mary’s Parish (Catholic Church with Spanish Mass and Puerto Rican congregation); - Catholic Charities (non-profit organization which has a heavy Puerto Rican clientele); . Mane Creations (Puerto Rican-owned beauty parlor frequented by Puerto Rican men and women); - Hispanic-American Store (Puerto Rican—owned and frequented); - Women, Infant and Child Program (VVIC) personal contact; - Key community persons at different work settings that have personal contacts with co-workers; - Annual census at the Town Clerk’s office. In addition to the above list, the researcher solicited family and friends to help “spread the word.” A letter explaining the purpose of the study was given to each of the community leaders listed in the above areas, and they were given instructions to ask their clients/parishnerslco-workers to participate by giving their address on an address “sign-up" sheet (see Appendix B). The community leader from the Head Start Program had access to several Puerto Ricans. Although she compiled nearly 35 names, she felt it best if the researcher mailed the surveys to her and that she administer the questionnaire to her clients. The researcher acquiesced, but still had to get the list of the names to (1) make sure that the respondents were not sent two surveys, and (2) know whether to send an English or Spanish questionnaire. The contact at the Head Start Program notified the researcher’s parents once 67 the questionnaires were completed so that the Head Start Program would not have to pay for their delivery. In addition to word of mouth and contacting community leaders, the researcher was granted the opportunity to address the Puerto Rican public at one of the heavily attended masses of the year: Christmas Eve Mass. The priest allowed the researcher to address the congregation. The speech was in Spanish (see Appendix B). The priest also situated a desk and chair for the researcher so that parishioners could sign up for the study after mass ended. Selsgion of subiscts: Problems and solutions to creating a list. While the method for the creation of a list is somewhat unorthodox, it is a functiOn of the population under study based on the researcher’s knowledge of the population. Therefore, alternative methods for a list were needed. Researchers call for creative solutions to this problem. For example, Salant and Dillman (1994) suggest creating a list from multiple sources or using a purposive sample design. Many Puerto Ricans in Southbridge do not have listed phone numbers, therefore, the phonebook was not trustworthy. Additionally, the Puerto Rican population is very mobile. Often times they will move one or two times a year, move in with extended family, or relocate to Puerto Rico. These situations create problems with using the telephone book as a list source. These Another traditional list source is the city’s annual census‘. This list has the same problems as phone listings. The town clerk mentioned that the census Under Massachusetts (MA) election law. MA cities are required to conduct an annual census. The census provides names, dates of birth, precinct and occupations. The annual census is used mostly for grants and public funding. 68 is not as accurate as they would like due to a lack of cooperation on the part of Puerto Ricans, undercounting, and the lack of Puerto Rican census takers (Helen I. Lenti, personal communication, December 22, 1998). Therefore, traditional sampling techniques need to be augmented to increase the possibility of an individual’s participation in the study. In an article on researching diverse populations, Henderson (1998) noted that “[methods] are important, but the strategies used to get information are essential. Researchers may need to stray from research protocol to obtain data and create an environment of social support.” While her comment was oriented towards qualitative research, it is appropriate for this study. Social support for this study was first created for the study to be successful in the adult Puerto Rican population. Knowledge of the population is critical to getting enough responses to perform useful analyses. The study population requires informal and formal lines of communication. For example, the researcher “informally” solicited names via personal contacts throughout the community in order to make Puerto Ricans aware of the study. It is culturally more acceptable to first “ask” if the subject’s name and address can be used for a mailing, and then perform the actual mailing. Watson (1992) noted that conventional sampling methods have been ineffective in reaching minority populations. He identified three sampling techniques and the problems associated with ethnic minorities. They are as follows: 69 1. Random sampling - inadequate as many in the ethnic community have not been on the electoral registers. 2. Quote sampling - insufficient data have existed from the census on which to sample targets and selected sample points may not reflect where ethnic groups actually are. 3. Random digit dialing - how do you establish ethnic origin with any degree of certainty by telephone? Maison, 1992, p. 339). Although his comments deal with ethnic minorities in the United Kingdom, researchers have encountered similar problems in the United States. Cox (1990), for example, argues for non-traditional designs and unconventional methods for researching minorities in the US. “Sutton and Schurrnan note that conventional methodology calls for all respondents and for investigators to refrain from disclosing the details of the research objective to the respondent. They found that both guidelines were unworkable in their own research [regarding minorities], however, and thus followed no rule. They acknowledge that they made a conscious decision to violate these rules despite the potential effects of bias on the results. They concluded that considerations such as an ability to obtain data and to create an environment of social support in which interviewees will provide responses must sometimes take precedence over traditional notion of scientific “objectivity.” I believe that their findings are applicable to other emotionally sensitive topics such as racioethnicity, and that they illustrate the need for new paradigms of research methodology (Cox, 1990, p. 11). The above quote illustrates several issues related to researching ethnic minorities. First, it is difficult to obtain truly representative samples of minority populations. Second, because of the difficulty in obtaining a list, other nontraditional methods are required to augment or create a list. Third, an environment of social support (over objectivity) is needed for participation. Through contact with community leaders, the researcher “spread the word” and Southbridge Puerto Ricans were more responsive. The social support was created along informal lines. Asking Puerto Ricans to fill out the 70 questionnaire when one first visits would be considered improper. An initial house call or visit should be informal. One can talk about business, but not actually conduct business. This is a similar concept to what Winter and Chavez (1998) referred to as taking time to “visit” for successful data gathering. If one were to conduct the personal interviews at the place of residence it would require at least two visits. The first visit would be to establish social support. This will typically involve sitting down for a cup of coffee, catch up on social, political, or family events in the community or Puerto Rico, and then discussion on the survey. To talk “just business” or visit quickly would be considered “rude” and would most likely not assure one of participation. A second visit would be required to conduct the actual interview where it would be considered an “official” visit. As one can gather, the cost and time for this method of ensuring an adequate sample size and response rate would be quite large. Pret tin h uestionnaire Two pretests were administered. The first pretest involved Puerto Ricans from Michigan State University’s (MSU) Puerto Rican Student Association. Because this particular group of Puerto Ricans were not from Southbridge, they provided excellent feedback on the wording of the questions (from English to Spanish), as well as remarks on the sensitivity of the questions being asked. In addition, the group of 10 MSU Puerto Ricans answered the questions regarding cultural identity, values, language preference, and perceived discrimination. 71 After evaluating the initial pretest, corrections were made, and the second pretest was administered to Puerto Ricans in Southbridge. From December 20, 1998 to January 4, 1999, the researcher met with 15 Southbridge Puerto Ricans and administered the questionnaire. Of the 15 Puerto Ricans in the pretest, eight questionnaires were administered in English and seven in Spanish. The following is a synopsis of the respondents: Mean time living in Southbridge: 21 years (Min: 2, Max: 35) 67% born in Puerto Rico, 33% in United States 33% Male, 67% Female Mean age: 35, (Minimum: 18, Maximum: 53) 40% Single, 53% Married, 7% Divorced 87% Employed, 7% Self-employed, 7% Other Each respondent was given the option of taking the survey in English or Spanish. The respondents were timed. After the respondents finished, the researcher asked the following questions: - Was there anything in the questionnaire you didn’t understand or found particularly difficult? - Were any of the questions offensive? - Do you think other Puerto Ricans would want to answer this survey? Why or why not? - Do you think the questionnaire is too long? Pretfit analysis of ssales. In assessing the reliability analysis the author took into consideration the N of 15. This is not a large enough N to warrant any conclusions, however it does provide some insight into the questions themselves. With an N of 15, the author expected anything above a .6 to be adequate. The alpha reliability is affected by the size of N, and by the number of items. In general, it is better to have more items so that if there were a need to delete one or two, one could still have a respectable scale. What follows is a 72 summary of the scales, their alpha reliabilities and the comments provided by the respondents‘from both pretests (See Appendix A for corresponding alpha-numerals in parentheses). Percein panefits of parks (IA-1G). The alpha reliability for the scale on perceived park benefits was .8783. The weakest measure was 1F. Without this item, the alpha reliability increased to .8880. Given this weak increase, and the low N, the researcher decided to retain the scale without any modifications. Respondents found these questions rather straight-fonrvard. Publis rgreation participation (Section 2A-2E). This is the dependent variable measuring park use. For this section, the author tried two different scales. The first scale for park use (not on the final version) utilized a scale that measured only the annual use of each park. The second scale divided the year into seasons. The decision of which scale to use was decided by (1) preference of the respondents, and (2) by the amount of information gathered by each of the different scales. In the first case, three of the respondents were visited twice (on two different days) by the researcher in order to see if the same information would be obtained and to see which scale was preferred by the respondents. In all three cases, the respondents preferred the “seasons” scale over the “annual” scale. In addition, the “season” scale offered more insight into actual use. In all three cases, the season scale gave more use. Respondents mentioned that it was easier to recall what they did by season than by year. Respondents also 73 mentioned that the use of “almost never” to “a lot“ above the categories was helpful. Park use was coded as: 0 = Never 1 = Once 3 = 2-4 times 9 = 5-13 times 17 = 14-20 times 25 = 21-29 times 30 = 30+ times The middle value of the number of times was given for each category. The seasons were then summed and a value was arrived for annual use for each park. The summation of all the parks is the “overall” park use value assigned to the respondent. The park which had the greatest use was Westville Dam (mean = 10.33, min: 0, Max: 52), followed by Henry Street Park (mean = 8.4, min: 0, max: 51). This was an interesting finding. Geographically, Westville Dam is furthest away from where the majority of Puerto Ricans live, and Henry Street is the closest to the Puerto Rican population. Acculturation sgle (Section 3A-3G and 4A-4D). The original wording is reflected in Table 3. The acculturation scale combined all the items from 3A to 40. However, it was found that the combined scale of language and values gave a .70 reliability. If one were to divide the two scales into language and values. The first acculturation scale (language) had an alpha reliability of .9280. Oddly enough, the weakest item was “preferred language at home.” With the removal of this item, the alpha reliability became .9452. It was decided that due to the small N, all items would be kept. 74 This second acculturation scale (values) tried to get at Puerto Rican versus American value systems. This was the weakest of all the scales and gave a -1.92 alpha reliability. This is an impossible number because the alpha reliability should be between 0 and 1. The researcher reverse-coded 4A and 4C because this reflected American values. If they are high on American values, then this should reflect a lower Puerto Rican valuation. With the inversion, the alpha reliability became .52. In the initial pretest, the items for the values scale were worded slightly different. The wording was changed for the second pretest because of recommendations from the MSU Puerto Ricans. However, a factor analysis showed two very distinct factors - an American and a Puerto Rican - on both pretests. It was decided to change all the items to reflect only Puerto Rican values because it seemed confusing to the respondents. Some respondents wondered why the researcher was asking on both American and Puerto Rican values, on the same issues. Although Cortes, et al. (1994) meant for the original scale to reflect biculturality, it seemed redundant to the respondents and it did not make sense to them. The change was made to reflect the feedback from both the analysis and the respondents. It was hypothesized that the change will create a stronger value scale. The combined scale of 11 items (language and values) for acculturation will be tested again in the actual study. If the two concepts do not converge into one scale, the researcher will use only the language items to measure acculturation because it is the stronger scale. 75 Parceivad discriminatipn (Section 5A-5E). The perceived discrimination scale had a reliability of .66. With the deletion of 5B the alpha became .70. With the deletion of 58 and 5A the alpha became .73. There were several issues regarding this particular scale. For example, statement 53 (see Table 5) was controversial with the respondents. Some questioned the relevance of the statement. However, when the researcher explained the reason for its inclusion, they agreed that it should be left in. The respondents felt that the “not sure” middle category should be changed to “neither agree nor disagree” as a better option. With this change, the researcher felt that it was acceptable to proceed with the scale and include both 5A and 5B in the scale. In addition, “feel” was include in the wording of all the items for the sake of consistency (compare the Table 5 wording with final wording in Appendix A). Subsgltural identfly (Section 6A-6F). The subcultural identity scale had a reliability of .58. With the deletion of 6B, the alpha reliability became .70 (see Table 4). Items 6B and 6E were re-coded in the reliability analysis. In the final cepy of the questionnaire, 6B remained the same, but the wording of GE seemed confusing to respondents because of the use of “not“ twice in the statement. As a result, SE was re-written so that it would not have to be re-coded. Although 63 may be too long, re-wording it would have been problematic because a shorter version would not give the same meaning in English or Spanish. If BB proved problematic during the actual study, it could always be taken out of the scale. 76 Dempgraphic section of pretest. There were two inconsistencies between the English and Spanish surveys on the last page of the survey. In the English survey, there was no “7A2" question which asked the respondent if helshe had ever lived in Puerto Rico, and if so how many years. This is getting at the acculturation issue and it is a counterpoint to how long they have lived in the United States. The second inconsistency involved the use of “total family income“ in the English version, and “individual income” in the Spanish version. It was an oversight on the part of the researcher, and was pointed out by a respondent who was editing the surveys for English/Spanish consistency in the translation. In addition, the income year was corrected; it was changed from 1997 to 1998. Below is a summary of the alpha reliabilities for each scale (with all items): Name pf Scale Alpha reliabil'sy Perceived Park Benefits .88 Language Preference .92 Values .52 Perceived Discrimination .66 Cultural Identity .58 The values scale when combined with the language preference scale yielded a .70 alpha reliability. This was to be a combined scale to measure acculturation as a reflection of language and values. The combined scale had a high reliability, but there may not be reason for combining them as recommended by previous research (Cortes, et al., 1994). The original scale version was weak on the “values” scale. The two scales listed above, which 77 were below .60, had items re-worded or changed entirely in order to make the statements more understandable. There was one final inconsistency regarding the demographics section. Items 7B3 and 7B4 asked for data concerning only one set of grandparents. The researcher did not correct this because it would have been problematic for the typesetting. As a result, this aspect of the study could not be incorporated in acculturation measures accounting for generational status. In addition, the socioeconomic scale was not tested because the researcher did not have the Hollingshead Index at the time of the pretest. Quastipnnaire distribution. The first step, discussed in previous sections, was to collect names via purposive and snow-ball sampling techniques to create a list. The reader should note that the researcher started to “spread the word” about the study about three weeks before the letters were sent to community leaders. The letters and sign-up sheets were sent to community leaders about a month prior to addressing the congregation on Christmas Eve. The second step was to cross-reference the names with the various lists to make sure that the names do not appear twice. The final list for the Puerto Rican adult population yielded 690 mailing addresses (539 signed up and 151 addresses were from the phone book and the town census), and 35 additional surveys to be delivered to the Head Start Program. There were 135 potential respondents who were Puerto Rican high schoolers. The total number of surveys to be distributed was 860 (62% of the 78 1397 resident Latinos aged 14 and over). The total time it took to collect the names and addresses was approximately two and a half months. The researcher opened “formal” lines of communication. Instead of making a “formal” second visit, adult subjects were contacted by mail. The surveys were mailed to those whose names were solicited. In addition, the cover letter should be made to look official by use of letterhead. It was expect that if Puerto Ricans voluntarily gave their names, and the study was being conducted by someone from their own community, they would be more inclined to participate. This approach was recommended by several leaders in the community. Coll ion Da In order to enhance a good response rate, the collection process included techniques suggested by Dillman (1978). Everything prescribed by Dillman’s total design method (T DM) was not followed due to monetary and time constraints. The basic concepts, however, were applied. The mailing procedure for the collection of the data involved the following steps: (1) mailing the introductory letter and autobiography; (2) mailing the cover letter and questionnaire; (3) mailing the follow-up letter and replacement questionnaire. The initial mailing involved the use of a letter and autobiography. The letter acted as an announcement and solicitation for completion of the questionnaire (See Appendix B). In addition, a short autobiography of the researcher was included so that Puerto Ricans had an update on the researcher since the time he left the community. The autobiography acted as a proxy for 79 the researchers’ personal visit, and allows for the respondent to have an idea of the researcher’s character. This is an extension of the social support concept. The initial mailing was sent by first class mail and used department envelopes and letterhead. One week after the initial mailing, the cover letter and questionnaire were mailed out (See Appendix B). The cover letter provided information on the purpose of the study, what the information will be used for, and how their names were chosen. In order to save on costs, the first wave of surveys were mailed by third class bulk mail. Accounting for a mailing time of 7-10 days, the return window given was approximately two weeks. There were some problems with the third class bulk mailing. After speaking with the post office in Southbridge, the researcher found that third class mail gets distributed very poorly and is not always sorted the same day it arrives because it is not considered priority mail. The first surveys were arriving haphazardly. As a result, the researcher waited an additional two weeks for responses to arrive before mailing the follow-up survey. In the cover letter, the researcher tried to convey an understanding of the problem with the mail, while at the same time expressing a necessity for having full participation. W The purpose of this study is to establish a substantive model and test it empirically. This model is a causal model. The hypotheses set forth in Chapter 1 reflect two things: (1) the proposed nature of the relationships between the theoretical constructs in the literature, and (2) the hypothesis that the data are 80 empirically consistent with the theoretical model. The strength of conducting a path analysis is that it can provide insight into the nature of the relationships between constructs. As a result, it illustrates where proposed causal relationships may not exist or at best very weak. Because the causal arrows in the model are “potential“ flows, if the data are not consistent with the model, the researcher can postulate an alternative model that is both empirically consistent with the data, and substantively consistent with the original theoretical underpinnings of the initial model as discussed in the literature review chapter. The data was analyzed with both a confirmatory factor analysis and a path analysis. The former assesses the validity and reliability of the indicators of the different constructs. The latter assesses the fit of the model. Step 1, than, is to run a confirmatory factor analysis on the total number of items and obtain the correlations among the constructs. Step 2 takes the correlations of the constructs as the basis for the path modeling. These steps will be discussed in more detail in the next chapter. Valig'mr and Reliabil'mr To the extent that the internal consistency theorem fits the data, this is a model of validity. The relationship between validity and reliability is conceptualized in a particular way. It does not make sense to calculate a reliability coefficient until one knows that all indicators are ultimate indicators of the same thing. A measure is reliable to the extent that it measures whatever it measures consistently. Reliability ranges, theoretically, from 0.0 to +1.0. Zero would mean 81 no reliability and one would mean perfect reliability. "What” it measures is a measure of validity, not a question of reliability. Reliability is based on an algorithm that tells one how consistent the measure is, but it says nothing about what one is measuring. There are several measures of reliability (e.g., test-retest, split-half), but the most often employed is Cronbach’s Alpha. The derivation of Cronbach’s Alpha is based on three assumptions: (1) that the unidimensional factor model is correct, (2) that the variances are equal, and (3) that all items are equally reliable (i.e., their factor loadings are the same). If the assumptions are wrong, then the alpha is an inaccurate assessment of the reliability coefficient. Robustness refers to the ability of an estimator to have its assumptions violated and still provide a useful estimates. Alpha is relatively robust with respect to the third assumption. However, If the unidimensional factor model is incorrect, the reliability coefficient does not make sense, it is a meaningless number. The reliability is something that is not a constnict. By employing a CFA, one can test the unidimensional factor for all possible parallel factors with the internal consistency and parallelism theorems. The first assumption - the idea that one has content validity - can be checked. The second assumption is that there are equal variances. That assumption is relaxed by standardizing the data. Standardizing the data forces all the variances to be equal. By forcing all the variances to be equal, the second assumption no longer applies. 82 The method of estimating reliability that is employed in this study, when we standardize all the variables, is Standardized Item Alpha. The Standardized Item Alpha is given in Program CFA”s output. It is the Speannan-Brown coefficient applied to the scaling problem and it is derived in the following manner: NF SI” = l+(N—1)F (1) The "F" is the mean of the correlations (which is an estimate of the reliability of one factor/construct). The 'N” is the number of indicators of the construct. If for example, there are four indicators, then that would mean that one is quadrupling the test. What this means, substantively and statistically, is that the more indicators a construct has, the higher the reliability (given unidimensionality) of the construct. Given the validity and reliability of one’s measures, one could safely sum indicators to create a composite variable (factor/construct) which could be used in a path analysis. Program CFA supplies the researcher with a factor correlation matrix corrected for attenuation. This is the matrix with the correlations among the constructs, which will be used in the path analysis. Assassment of Errors/Testing for Fit A final word on residuals is necessary for the reader to fully comprehend the next section. Errors are assessed in order to find whether the indicator is within sampling error of zero. As such, the null hypothesis is that error equals 83 zero. In other words, one wants to fail to reject (accept) the null hypothesis and find it NOT significant, i.e., no significant error. If one is assessing errors (i.e., testing the fit of the model) using the significance test, one is looking for a probability value greater than or equal to .05 (when testing at the 95% confidence interval) in the output. Program CFA uses the chi square test of fit to assess the fit of the model. The chi square test reverses the role of the null hypothesis. The research hypothesis is the null hypothesis. Therefore, a low chi square is good. A large chi square indicates departure from the model. If chi square is not significant, than the model fits. An alternative to the significance test is to compare the observed versus the expected correlations for the items (during both the internal consistency and parallelism tests) and determine whether the errors are within sampling error of zero by using the confidence interval approach. A combination of both methods for assessing the errors are employed in this study, with greater emphasis placed on the confidence interval approach. Chapter 4 DATA ANALYSIS This chapter is partitioned into three distinct sections for the purpose of clarity. The first section is an overview of the descriptive statistics regarding the survey respondents. Section two addresses the findings of the CFA on the study data. Of particular interest in this section are the items which were used in the final indicas. The last section of this chapter examines the path analysis, and assesses the fit of the model. Section One: Dessriptiva Statistiss W8: A total of 690 questionnaires were mailed to Puerto Ricans in Southbridge (see Table 7). Of the 690 surveys initially mailed, 77 (11%) were returned due to incorrect addresses. The majority (45) of the addresses for the returned letters were addresses from the phonebook. Thirty percent of the phonebook address were incorrect, while only five percent of the sign-up list yielded incorrect addresses. This reinforces the notion that purposive sampling was indeed a better way to identify the desired respondents than using the telephone directory, especially given the mobile nature of the population under study. A total of approximately 613 questionnaires was probably delivered. A total of 304 Puerto Ricans responded by mail. This produced a response rate of approximately 50%. The low level of response was probably a result of the third class postage for the first mailing of the questionnaires. Factors which may have influenced the overall response included the following: 85 . lack of priority given to third class mail; . lack of current addresses in phone book; . no forwarding address. Because of problems related to mailing and delivery, the response rate of 50% is a conservative estimate. The amount of actual delivered questionnaires is unknown. Table 7 Mailed Surveys and Response Sam le Initial lncorr Prob I Source Mailed A r 33 Dalivered Phone Book 151 45 106 Sign-up List 52$ 32 5_07_ Totals 690 77 Completed 8 Returned by mail: 304 (50% response rate) High School: 57 Community Leader". 29 Unusable Surveys: :6 Total usable surveys (N) - In addition to the 304 returned by mail, 29 were received from the community leader at the Head Start Program, and 57 came from Southbridge High School (T able 7). The number of high school students who responded could have been higher as well. Accessibility to all 135 Latino high schoolers was not possible for several reasons: questionnaires were distributed one day only (during home room period); - questionnaires were distributed the first day after students returned from Spring Break, and attendance may have been lower that day; 86 eligible students could have been late or sick, and; . not all Latino high schoolers are Puerto Rican. On the day the questionnaire was administered, there were 57 Puerto Rican students in the high school during home room period. Six of the surveys were unusable because of incomplete or ambiguous answers, or because the respondents were not Puerto Rican. The total amount of usable surveys (N) totaled 384. Of the 384 surveys, 209 (54%) were in Spanish. Nearly 61% of the adult population preferred the questionnaire in Spanish, and 73% of all respondents were born in Puerto Rico. Sampla and Pppulation Dampgraphics Because there is no information on non-response, the sample was compared to population figures from the 1990 US. Census to assess representativeness5. Puerto Ricans constitute 90% of the Latino population in Southbridge. The researcher used Latinos (Hispanics) as the reference population when comparing the sample to the population. The 1990 Census does not have a breakdown by Puerto Ricans. According to the 1990 Census, persons 14 years of age and older, of Latino origin, constitute 1,397 total persons. The mean age of respondents in the study is 34, with the youngest respondent being 14 years of age, and the oldest being 80 years of age. The 1990 Census Latino high school age group (14-17 year olds) for Southbridge is made up of 129 persons. This corresponds to the 135 Latinos at Southbridge There are differences beMen the sample and the 1990 population statistics. However, one may speculate whether acensusthatisnearlyadecadeoldistrulyrepresentative. 87 Hig Stu Inc High School. The study does not, however, include high school drop outs. This study includes 42% (57) of the high school age Latinos. In addition, this study includes 27% (384 of 1,397) of the entire Latino population of Southbridge. Table 8 illustrates the frequencies between age and gender in the sample, and expected frequencies based on census percentages. In both cases, the observed frequencies do not equal the expected frequencies. The chi square for age is 61.92 08m (0,: _05. a: ,0, = 18.31 ). Chi square for gender is 31-29 (xzcrltlcal (an .05, dl= 1) = 334)- Table 8 A nd Gend rBr kdown in nsus an am I wt 0 N= 1 Samm “ Census ° (°/°) (W 1 . 14-1 7 14 9 2. 1 8-21 8 12 3. 22-24 5 9 4. 25-29 14 16 5. 30-34 1 5 17 6. 35-39 10 13 7. 40-44 10 9 8. 45-49 9 5 9. 50-54 5 3 10. 55-59 5 4 1 1. 60+ 5 3 Gend r Cat 0 N=3 2 Sample“ ansus " (°/°) (‘70) 1. Male 40 54 2. Female 60 46 a - Survey respondents. b - From 1990 Census 88 The sample is over-represented in the 14-17 age group and in ages 40 through 54, and 60 and over. The sample is under-represented between the ages of 22 through 39. Nearly 60% of the respondents are over the age of 30. This suggests that a relatively older group of residents were sampled. Table 9 shows the educational attainment for persons 25 and over of Latino origin in the sample. The observed frequencies do not equal the expected frequencies. The observed chi square is 58.13 (sz (a. .05. d: a, = 12.59). Table 9 shows that the sample is generally over-representative of education higher than a high school diploma. Half the respondents attained a high school diploma or some college. These data suggest a wide range of educational attainment. Table 9 Education for Persons 25 and Over of Latino ri in in ensus and Sam Ie Egucation Categog (N=271) Sam Ie' Census " (%) (%) 1. Less than 9‘” grade 22 32 2. Some high school 13 24 3. High school diploma 33 21 4. Some college, no degree 17 13 5. Associate’s degree 9 4 6. Bachelor’s degree 3 4 7. Graduate school 3 2 a - Survey respondents. b - From 1990 Census 89 Univariata Analysis. Appendix A offers a breakdown of the distribution of responses for each variable in the study. The check boxes in the English survey were replaced by percentages. Means, standard deviations, and the range was provided for non- categorical variables. Ancestry. In this sample, 73% of Southbridge Puerto Ricans were born in Puerto Rico. In addition, nearly 100% of all Puerto Ricans had parents which were both Puerto Rican. There are almost no respondents of mixed ancestry. This finding suggests a homogenous ethnic group. In addition, the majority of respondents either emigrate or are of first generation status. The data also suggest that cultural ties are not only reinforced by familial ties, but by ties to the island of Puerto Rico. Neany 80% of the respondents have lived in Puerto Rico. When comparing the average years (18.5) living in Puerto Rico and in the United States (19.5) the means are very similar. The standard deviations and the minimum and maximum years are also similar (see Appendix A). This supports the view that there are strong ties to the island and that Puerto Ricans in Southbridge can be generalized to other Puerto Ricans. In addition, one could speculate that there is continuous migration back and forth between Puerto Rico and Southbridge. Agulturation ans subcultural identfl' . Because the acculturation processes and cultural identity may play a role in recreation, the means of time spent living in Puerto Rico by age cohort was examined. Table 10 is instructive for several reasons. There is a significant difference between the means of the 90 various age groups (F=16.94, df = 11, ci= .000). The means show a positive relationship between one’s generation or age (indicative of acculturation) and time spent living in Puerto Rico. Table 10 ANOVA of Years_i_n Puerto Rico by Age Cohort _Aga N 2am Mean gs, Minimum Maximum Qohort omen Deviation 14-17 32 57 6.22 4.53 1 15 18-21 17 53 10.71 6.59 1 20 22-24 12 66 7.42 5.16 1 19 25-29 35 66 14.43 7.00 2 26 30-34 46 80 17.57 9.08 1 32 35-39 29 76 18.69 10.14 1 37 40-44 35 88 22.91 9.72 1 38 45-49 34 100 24.53 8.70 10 40 50-54 19 95 25.89 9.80 14 41 55-59 14 100 27.36 10.30 16 47 __60+ 21 100 29.31 13.02 15 59 Total 294 77 18.54 11.14 1 59 Sum of Sguarg S1! Mean [3 _S_ig._ . Sguare Between Groups 14481.701 11 1316.518 16.944 .000 Within Groups 2191 1.224 282 77.699 Total 36392.925 293 The percent of all respondents in each age cohort that have lived in Puerto Rico is decreasing by each newer generation. For example, 53% to 57% of pre-adult (ages 14-21) respondents have lived in Puerto Rico. This compares to 66% for young adults in their twenties; 76% to 88% who are middle age (ages 30-44); and 95% to 100% of respondents over age 45 who have lived in Puerto 91 Rico. The descriptive evidence suggests that this study sample is reflective of the population. The implication of this finding, with respect to the EPRP Model, is that it forecasts a strong relationship between acculturation and subcultural identity. A limitation of this finding is that there is no data which would indicate what period of their lives was spent in Puerto Rico. Knowledge of the period spent in Puerto Rico might shed light on the relationship between acculturation, subcultural identity, and discrimination. Park use. The average park use was 74 visits per year to Southbridge public parks (N=383). The park used the most was Henry Street ()1 = 24.01 ), followed by Westville Dam (52 = 18.38), McMahon Field (52 = 13.91 ), Morris Street (x = 8.77), and West Street (>7 = 8.48). Appendix B provides a map of Southbridge and the spatial distribution of its parks. Although age was not incorporated in the current model, there can be significant difference in park use between students and adults. In the study, Puerto Rican high school students have mean annual park visits of 124 visits per person (N=57), compared with Puerto Rican adults who averaged 65 visits to public parks (N=326). There is a relationship between park visits and whether or not the visitor is a student or an adult ()8: 7.01, dfi1, or: .008, 2-tail). There is also a significant difference between the means of student and adult park use (t = -6.03, df = 381, or: .000, 2-tail). It can be argued that age or cohort could be a factor on park use. 92 The researcher investigated whether students differed from adults (on average) with respect to their views on the benefits of parks. Table 11 indicates that there is no difference between students and adults in terms of how each group views the benefits of parks as; (1) a place to for exercising, (2) a place to socialize, (3) a place for kids to go, or (4) a place for providing open space. Adults and students did differ on their perceptions of parks as a place for (1) escape and (2) enjoyment, with adults showing stronger agreement. There was barely a significant difference between students and adults on their perception of parks as a place where family and friends could meet. The two top ranked perceived benefits of going to a park by both groups were that (1) parks provide a place for kids to go, and (2) parks provide a place where family and friends meet. This is consistent with Huthison’s (1987) and Chavez’ (1993) observations that Latinos are very family oriented. Table 11 Mean Difference in PQEIVQQ Banefits pf Parks Between Students and Adults Perceiv Tptal Studant Agult 1 g a Banefrts Maan Maan Mean (twp;tail) Enjoy nature 4.06 3.58 4.15 4.20 380 0.00 Escape 4.12 3.79 4.18 2.90 380 0.00 Socialize/contacts 3.95 4.07 3.93 -0.99 372 0.32 Exercise 4.08 3.88 4.12 1 .82 378 0.07 Family/friends 4.33 4.1 1 4.36 2.08 378 0.04 Open Space 4.09 4.09 4.10 0.06 379 0.95 For kids to go 4.46 4.46 4.46 0.04 380 0.96 93 Based on the researchers understanding of the Puerto Rican culture, the Puerto Ricans in Southbridge are very family oriented. A strong family oriented park atmosphere that is conducive to family and children-oriented activities would be draw for Puerto Ricans. Henry Street park is the most visited park by Southbridge Puerto Ricans. This is probably due to several reasons: . it is the closest park to where the majority of Puerto Ricans reside; - structural changes (playground improvements) were made to accommodate more children; . cultural (Puerto Rican festivals) and family-oriented activities were targeted at Puerto Ricans; 0 Henry Street Park has three baseball diamonds (the most for any park in Southbridge); . the tennis courts at Henry Street Park were changed to basketball courts. These sports, baseball and basketball, are the number one and number two sports, respectively, among Puerto Ricans. Westville Dam, however, is the furthest from the majority of Puerto Ricans in Southbridge. This park may be popular and have high visitation because it is very unique. It is the only recreation area that has a river running through it. This offers Puerto Ricans the opportunity to fish. Westville Dam is the only place that has grills and picnic tables which are appealing to family oriented Latinos (Hutchison, 1988) . Many church outings are also held there. The reader should not make light of these findings. Puerto Ricans hold their families (immediate and extended) at the core of their culture. Ties along familial lines are very strong. The surprising finding was the use of McMahon Field. Further inspection reveals a large discrepancy between mean use by students (39.09) and mean use by adults (9.52) which is reflective of its location. McMahon Field is located 94 across the street from Southbridge High School, and it is where most high schoolers ”hang out“. It is also located in a more affluent part of Southbridge where less Puerto Ricans live. In addition, the park is unique because it offers the only track field and football field in the city. As a final note, mean use of parks is misleading because it does not take into account seasonal variation for each park. The distribution of the frequency of use by season for each park (see Appendix A) shows that the mean is not a good indicator of use. Lack of use in the winter, spring and fall months. However if one looks at only the Summer, public parks are used more widely and between 50% and 90% of respondents use the parks in some capacity. D ta in and lean-u The initial run of the correlation matrix between all the factors gave a lower N than expected. The majority of housewives, students and retired persons were left out of the analysis because they were not coded for occupation. Following the coding for the Hollingshead Index, housewives were coded "0" on their occupational score, students were coded "1” for their occupation score, and retired persons were coded “2”. Excluding cases listwise, the final N used for the CFA and path analysis was 311. This included the housewives, students and retired persons. In addition, not all items were used in the initial CFA run. Income was not used in the socioeconomic construct because of reasons listed earlier. The socioeconomic construct had two indicators: education and occupation. Additionally, questionnaire item 6B (see Appendix A) was not used 95 as an indicator for the cultural identity construct. During the pilot test, this item seemed problematic and it was kept after it was re-worded. However, the item continued to present problems in the consistency of responses to it. The item was not used because it was long and unclear. Lastly, the items for Acculturation (3A-3G, see Appendix A) were re-coded so that ”1" reflected ”only Spanish” and "5” reflected "only English". The researcher realized that the more Spanish-oriented the respondent was, the less helshe was acculturated. Therefore, the stronger one’s preference towards Spanish, the lower that respondent should be in their acculturation process. Section Twp: Cpnfimiatppr Factor Analysis The model to be tested consists of six constructs: (1) Acculturation (ACC), (2) Subcultural Identity (SID), (3) Socioeconomic Status (SES), (4) Perceived Benefits of Parks (BEN), (5) Perceived Discrimination (DIS), and (6) Recreation Participation (REC). The testing of this model will be done one construct at a time. The initial CFA began with 35 items. This created a 35x35 matrix yielding 595 correlations. The N for the CFA is 311. Three criteria are used to assess the fit of the model: (1) face validity, (2) internal consistency, (3) parallelism. Face validity was assessed in the previous chapter and will not be discussed further in this analysis. It is assumed, based on the results of the two pilot studies, and the literature review, that all items meet the criterion for face validity. The qualitative and substantive evidence suggest that all the items measure their respective constructs. This data analysis concerns itself with the latter criteria and addresses each accordingly with respect to each construct. 96 lntemal Consistensy lntemal consistency examines how the items in a cluster (construct) relate to each other (see Appendix C). The pattern of interrelationships should be consistent with the hypothesis that all items measure the same construct. This will be considered for each construct that has more than four indicators. Constructs having less than four indicators cannot be tested for lntemal consistency without making some assumptions. Unless otherwise noted, the lntemal consistency tests allow for a gradient in the correlation matrix (allows for variation in item quality, i.e., the factor loadings are not equal). Psrceivfi benefits of parks. The initial BEN construct started with seven indicators (see Items 1-7 in Table 13). The value of chi square was 62.42 with a p-value of .00. The chi square is not near zero, and the p-value is not above .05. This implies that the construct is not internally consistent. The size of the errors for the original seven items were then assessed by looking at the item with the largest errors (sum across the row for each item in the correlation matrix). Next, the researcher observed what happened to the sums when indicators were removed. If questionably large errors remain, the last check is to see if the observed correlation is within the expected correlation’s confidence interval for those correlations whose errors are large. In Table 12, the obtained correlations are located in the bottom left triangle of the first matrix, and the predicted correlations on the upper right triangle in bold. Predicted correlations were derived by employing the product 97 rule and multiplying the factor loadings (designated “FL” in Table 12) for the corresponding items. For example, the predicted correlation between Item 1 and Item 2 is the product of the factor loadings for each item (.66 * .66 = .44). The absolute difference between the obtained and predicted is then put in the error matrix. Table 12 Assessing Errors for BEN Const ct Obtained/Predicted Correlations Error Matrix 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 )1 1 .44 .42 .50 .53 .46 .40 1 .34 2 .59 .42 .50 .53 .46 .40 2 * . . . . .34 3 .40 .43 .48 .51 .45 .38 3 .02 .01 ** .00 .01 .00 .01 .05 4 .51 .47 .43 .6 .52 .45 4.01 .03 .00 ** .03 .00 .03 .10 5 .4353 .52 .57 .5543 5.05 .00 .01 .03 ** .07 .01 .17 6 .45 .40 .45 .52 .56 .42 6 -0 .06 00-00-07 “” -06 20 7 .30 _31 .39 .43 .55 .49 7 f .01 .03 .01 .06 ** .30 FL .66 .66 .64 .75 .80 .70 .60 Looking at the error matrix in Table 12, one can see that Items 1, 2, and 7 have large errors associated with them. For a more thorough assessment, the researcher viewed the confidence interval around the predicted correlation between Item 2 and Item 7 (the lowest of the high correlations): p (.31 $.40 $.49) = .95. The obtained correlation of .31 is barely in the confidence interval, indicating that errors of .09 will not make this model fit the data. 98 Therefore, Items 1 (Enjoy nature at parks), 2 (Parks allow me to escape), and 7 (Parks are a place for kids to go) were discarded. The modified BEN construct consisting of Items 3, 4, 5, and 6 (see Table 13) yielded a chi square of .306. The p-value was .998. The chi square is close to zero, the p-value is greater than .05, and the errors are within sampling error of zero. The BEN construct passed the internal consistency test with four indicators and it is unidimensional (all items measure the same construct). Table 12 illustrates the process which is repeated for each construct. The significance test aside, each construct was submitted to the same process of obtaining correlations, comparing them to predicted correlations, and assessing the errors. As such, the obtained/predicted correlations and the error matrix for each construct will not be shown. The reader should use Table 12 as a model. Agulturatipn. The initial ACC construct consisted of 11 indicators, Items 8-18 (see Table 13). The original conception of the ACC construct consisted of both language and value variables. The 11-indicator ACC construct had a chi square value of 1217.53 and a p-value of .00. The enormous chi square and p- value indicated significantly huge errors. The factor loadings for Items 15-18 were very different in size from the factor loadings of Items 8-14. This indicates that perhaps there are two constructs. The first construct measuring acculturation in terms of language preference, and the second construct measuring acculturation in terms of values. Assessment of the error matrix showed that Items 15-18 contained large errors, with individual item residuals having errors over .30. Items 15-18 were 99 omitted from the construct cluster and errors were assessed based on seven items (Items 8-14). These seven items reflected acculturation based on language preference and use. The factor loadings were re-estimated and the chi square of the 7-indicator ACC construct was 252.16 with a p-value of .00. Again, the chi square was considerably large and the construct had significant errors. Assessment of the correlation matrix led to the omission of Items 9, 10, and 13 (see Table 13). Item 13 had the most error associated with it. Item 9 had the second most error and did not match the strength of the factor loadings. Item 10’s largest error did not fall within the confidence interval of the predicted correlation. This left the ACC construct with four indicators. The abridged scale yielded a chi square of 10.04, with a p-value of .07. The errors are within sampling error of zero with this set of indicators, and the significance test states that the errors are not significant. The ACC construct is internally consistent and therefore unidimensional. Perceived dissrimination. The DIS construct initially consisted of five indicators, Items 19-23 (see Table 13). The five items yielded a chi square of 41.07 with a p-value of .00. The original construct had significant errors. Assessment of the residual matrix showed that Items 19 and 20 had the same summed total of errors. 100 Table 13 Descriptive Statistig For ltams Used in the CFA (N = 311) Standard Mean Construct Deviation Perceived Benefits c .9 .. E :3 +_- g Perceived Discrimination Subcultural Identity Public Recreation economic ' are " - Shaded items used in final confirmatory factor analysis. ‘ - Shaded items passed tests for lntemal consistency and parallelism. 101 When each item was individually removed from the scale to assess how each affected the factor loadings, the factor loadings were more uniform with Item 19, than with Item 20. In addition, Item 20 did not match the initial factor loading gradient, and it’s highest error did not fall within the predicted correlation’s confidence interval. Item 20 was removed from the scale. The four indicators measuring DIS were Items 19, 21, 22, and 23. This yielded a chi square of 6.84 with a p-value of .23. The errors are not significantly different from zero and the construct is internally consistent yielding a unidimensional model. Subcultural ident’ny. SID was measured by five indicators, Items 24-28 (see Table 13). The chi square for the five indicator model was 31.03 with a p- value of .00. The model was not internally consistent and had significant errors. Assessment of the errors disclosed Item 28 as having the most error associated with it. Item 28 also failed the confidence interval test. With the deletion of Item 28, the SID construct’s chi square became .56 (which is very close to zero), with a probability of .99. This indicates almost perfect internal consistency and one can conclude that the four indicators are indeed measures of the same construct. The construct is unidimensional. Public racreation. The REC construct consisted of five indicators, Items 29-33 (see Appendix C). The five items yielded a chi square of 42.39, with a p- value of .00. The initial REC construct was not unidimensional. Assessment of the residual matrix found that absolute errors larger than .09 would place the obtained correlation outside of the predicted correlation’s 102 confidence interval. Item 29 had two larger errors than .09 (.11 and .13). With the deletion of Item 29 from the set of indicators, the chi square became 6.19 with a probability of .29. The four items did not have errors significantly different from zero. The REC construct is unidimensional. Socioeconomic status. SES could not be tested for internal consistency because it only has two indicators. A construct with less than four indicators is under-identified. This means that one cannot estimate the parameters unless one makes an assumption, mainly that the parameters (factor loadings) are both equal. One cannot test the fit of the model (lntemal consistency) with an under- identified status. Test of Parallelism After the internal consistency analysis, there were 23 total indicators. There are 253 correlations among 23 indicators. One should expect 12 or 13 errors to be large by chance alone. The biggest error found in the matrix was .23. The correlation associated with that is -.09. The sample size is 311. The standard error is .06. That means that the 95% confidence interval around that correlation is :t .12. The reader should remember that this is in the parallelism blocks of the matrix. Parallelism is calculated using three correlations, all of which are subject to sampling error. Given this, an error of .14 may not be so unusual Errors that were .15, or above, were considered large given the sample size of 311 and the sampling error associated with the three correlations used in estimation. There were seven correlations which were .15 or above, and four of 103 the highest (.16, .17, .19, and .23) were associated with Item 33 (Harry J. McMahon Field). Through inspection of the correlation matrix alone, Item 33 would not pass the parallelism test because of the high amount of errors associated with it and items in other constructs. The researcher inspected the original correlation matrix and noted that Item 33 correlates higher with other items in other constructs. This is a violation of parallelism. Therefore, Item 33 was deleted from the analysis. The final parallelism check was conducted on 22 items. The next step is to perform a CFA and test for parallelism with the remaining 22 items. The test for parallelism is quite involved. A test of parallelism for the SES construct will be presented to illustrate the process. After the SES example, a parallelism synopsis for all constructs will be provided. The parallelism theorem states that the correlation between items of two different (parallel) constructs should equal the triple product of the factor loadings for each item and the correlation between the constructs. To the extent that this does not hold true, there is error. The test of significance for the parallelism theory tests the idea that the errors are within sampling error of zero, i.e., the errors are not significantly different from zero. However, more important than the significance test is the analysis of the errors. §E§ Qpnstgig as an exampla pf parallelism. Table 14 presents the factor loadings and factor correlations (from Program CFA output) needed to obtain the predicted correlation between items of different constructs in order to perform the parallelism test. To check whether the items for SES are parallel to the 104 items for BEN one assesses the correlations between the items for each construct. Table 14 Item py Factor and Factor by Factor Correlation Matrix ' ” BEN A C - -DIS ID REC SES 3 4 5 I3 8 liifiTZ '14 19 I21 22 I23 24 I25 26 I27 30 31 32 34 35 Le» lssjsrs I.» I." -.31 -.02 .15 l .38 [-27 Incc -.31 .14 I." I." I.» -.48 -.69 .09 .27 DIS '02 --48 .70 Isa so I.” ' 53 -.12 -.16 SID .15 -.69 .53 .54 I.» lsrz [.ss .05 -.26 REC .38 .09 -.12 .05 .52 I.“ .si -.10 p53 -.27 .27 -.16 -.26 -.10 e... I... - - the factor loadings of each item on each factor (construct) is in bold/smaller font -. the correlations between each factor (construct) is in normal font (off-diagonal) The following is the obtained correlations between the items for SES (Items 34 and 35) and BEN (Items 3, 4, 5, and 6): 3.4. -.18 -.09 -.16 -.04 I0) I01 I-h I0) 35 -.16 -.13 -.22 -.20 The predicted correlations from the parallelism theory are obtained by multiplying the factor loading for the first item by the factor loading for the second item, and multiply this product by the correlation between the constructs. 105 For example, to obtain the predicted correlation between Item 3 and Item 34, one multiplies Item 3’s factor loading (.65) by Item 34's factor loading (.75) by the correlation between BEN and SES (-.27). The triple product [(.65)(.75)(—.27)] is -.13. One does this for each of the correlations. This yields the following predicted correlations and corresponding errors: Pgdicted Correlations: Absolute Errors: a 35 a 352 3 -.13 -.13 _3 .05 .03 5 -.15 -.15 _4 .06 .02 a -.16 -.16 5 .00 .06 _6 -.14 -. 14 _6 .10 .06 The errors are not large and one can conclude, by the assessment of the errors, that the items for BEN (Items 3, 4, 5, and 6) are parallel to the items for SES (Items 34 and 35). This operation is performed for every set of items on every construct. The next step is to see if the SES items are parallel with respect to the ACC items, and so on. Appendix D presents the final 22 item correlation matrix used to assess parallelism. It illustrates the errors in the lower left portion of the matrix and the predicted correlations in the upper right corner of the matrix. Once SES is assessed for parallelism among all the other factors, a “total” parallelism measure is computed by Program CFA. This is the extent to which SES (or any other construct) is parallel to all other constructs. It is also a chi square test and is interpreted in the same manner as the chi square for the lntemal consistency theorem. The null hypothesis is that error is equal to zero. If one fails to reject chi square, then the items in the construct are not affecting 106 the items in the other constructs. Table 15 provides the chi square analysis of the parallelism given by Program CFA. Table 15 Parallelism Ana sis Construct th Sguare P-Value Gradient/Flat BEN (Items 3.4.5.6) 11.20 .74 Gradient ACC (Items 8,11,12,14) 22.62 .09 Flat DIS (Items 19, 21.22.23) 23.05 .08 Gradient SID (Items 24.25.26.27) 23.88 .07 Flat REC (Items 30.31.32) 16.94 .08 Flat SES (Items 34.35) 9.02 .11 Gradient The parallelism theorem held for all constructs (T able 15). The p-values are larger than .05. Therefore. the errors are not significantly different from zero. The model fits. In addition to the significance tests, the errors for all the correlations in the off-diagonal cells of the 22-item correlation matrix (parallelism blocks) were within sampling error of zero. Thus the test for parallelism held for all items and all constructs. Once the constructs pass the internal consistency and parallelism tests. they are considered valid and can be summed to create a composite variable. The reliabilities of each of the constructs is given in Table 16. The next section discusses the steps taken in the path analysis to test the fit of the causal model. 107 Table 16 R Ii bil’ An sis Construct Standardized Item Alpha BEN (Items 3,4,5,6) .81 ACC (Items 8,11,12,14) .89 DIS (Items 19, 21.22.23) .83 SID (Items 24.25.26.27) .70 REC (Items 30.31.32) .63 SES (Items 34,35) .72 Section Three: Path Analysis on EPRP Model In the path analysis, the constructs are now considered (composite) variables in the EPRP Model. Program Path is the software program used to perform the statistics for the path analysis. It was initially developed by Hunter (May 3, 1984) and revised by Hunter and Hamilton (August 17, 1997). The main input for Program Path is the correlation matrix (taken from Program CFA) and the specification of the path model. From this, Program Path’s main output is the path analysis itself which gives the original correlation matrix. the reproduced correlation matrix, the error matrix and the chi square and p-value. Path analysis is based on a set of structural equations. The precise mathematics behind path analysis will not be discussed here. For a more comprehensive explanation of the mathematics behind causal models, see Duncan (1975), Heise (1975), Kenny (1979), Kline (1998), and Loehlin (1987). It is important, however, to have a basic understanding of the concepts involved 108 with path analysis in order to understand this section and the interpretation of the Program Path output. Path AnaNsis Qongms Path models use standardized data. Therefore, the path coefficients are equivalent to regression coefficients. The path model is a null hypothesis. What one is testing is that the data are consistent with the model, i.e., the errors associated with the model are equal to zero. Over-jdentificatipn status. In order to evaluate the model, one has to estimate the parameters (path coefficients) and assess the fit of the model. To estimate the parameters. the model must be over-identified. The identification status of the model refers to the amount of information that one has in the data set relative to the amount of information one has to estimate the parameters. A model is over-identified when the number of correlations exceed the number of parameters one has to estimate. Simply put, subtract the number of paths in the model from the number of total possible correlations and that is the identification status. Total number of possible correlations between variables is derived by using the following formula: [CZ—k (2) 2 Where “k” is equal to the number of variables in the model. In the case of the proposed EPRP Model, there are six variables. Using the formula, there are 15 correlations to estimate. 109 Next, one looks at the number of arrows (paths) in the model. There are 10 paths. The number of paths is the number of correlations that are constrained (used up in estimating the path coefficients) to equal their obtained value by definition. Where ever there is a direct effect postulated, that correlation is used for estimating the parameters. The remaining five correlations (meme... News, rAccmEC, [9.0553, and rams) are not used up in estimating the parameters (see Figure 11). Their predicted value is NOT constrained to equal their obtained value. These five parameters are what one has to estimate. These are the correlations that are to be tested to assess the fit of the model, i.e., it leaves five degrees of freedom which can be used to test the fit of the model. If the model fits, the observed minus the predicted value (errors) should be equal to zero. Visualizing the correlation matrix. As one adds variables to a causal string, the correlation between the first variable in the causal string and the last variable will get progressively smaller as a result of the product rule. The relevance of this is that if one orders the variables in the correlation matrix according to their causal order, the correlations should decrease as they leave the diagonal of the matrix. This is a diagnostic tool for assessing errors and evaluating the logic of the model and its resultant fit. 110 Figure 11. The EPRP Model (with non-constrained paths) ”“3 Recreation Participation Local vsrsus global test. The five non-constrained correlations are the basis for local tests of the model. Program Path calls the local tests the individual link analysis. This is the test that examines whether the errors for each non-constrained link is within sampling error of zero. The error obtained for each local test, divided by the standard error of the difference is distributed as a z-value. One then uses the 2 test to examine the hypothesis that the errors are within sampling error of zero. If the z-value is less than -1.96. or greater than 1.96, then it is a large error at the .05 level. It is statistically significant at .05 (it should not be statistically significant, i.e., not statistically different from zero). If 2 is not as low as -1.96 or higher than 1.96, then the error is not statistically different from .05. This is the 2 test and it is performed for each of the five links, giving us five 111 different standard errors. Program Path computes each of these standard errors. The global test is the test of the model as a whole. The global test is distributed as a chi square. Chi square by definition is the distribution that is the sum of squared normal deviates. In this case, there are five normal deviates (five z-values). Program Path squares each of the five z-values and adds them up, yielding a chi square with five degrees of freedom. The chi square should not be statistically significant, if the model is to fit. The importance of the local and global tests is that. if the model does not fit the data, the tests allow one to see where the model failed. The model as a whole could work because the chi square is a summed value, but perhaps one or more of the links are faulty. It is possible to have no errors on four of the links and have a huge error on one link. The global test would not pick this up“. Correlations and path coefficients. The final concept that one should understand for path analysis is the difference between a correlation and a path coefficient. The correlations between constructs include direct. indirect and spurious relationships. The path coefficient, however, is the measure of direct effects only. The path coefficients control for indirect and spurious effects. This is the advantage of Program Path over LISREL. LISREL does not give the local tests. it only gives the global test It also does not give a significance test using corrected coefficients. Using uncorrected correlations. the model falls every time (Frank J. Boster, personal communication, February 9, 1999). LISREL uses maximum likelihood estlrnation, while Program Path uses ordinary least squares. Although maximum likelihood is a more efficient estimate. it spreads the error across the correlation matrix. As a result, one can never figure out where one went wrong. Ordinary least squares localizes the error. It is important to look at both the local and global tests to see where the model could be improved. The most important step Is to diagnose the errors. The significance test is a slrnple decision rule, but the errors are what really matter. 112 The Path AnaNsis The first step in the path analysis was to estimate the parameters for the non-constrained links in the proposed EPRP Model. The corrected correlation matrix used for the Program Path analysis is shown in Table 17. Although Program CFA corrects for the correlations among the constructs, it does not correct for attenuation in the correlations among the indicators. The researcher used SPSS 9.0 for \MndowsO to compute the uncorrected correlation matrix between the constructs. Using the Iistwise option, the matrix with the correlations among the constructs yielded an N of 318 for the 22 items that make up the constructs. When the correlations went from 35 items to 22 items, the sample increased. The path analysis, therefore, is based on an N of 318. Table 17 Obtained Corralatign Matrix for Path Analysis (N=318) .899. fig §_E_§ 91$ BEN BED M 1 .00 -.69 -.29 -.48 -.34 .09 §l_Q -.69 1.00 -.27 .55 .16 .08 §_E_S .29 -.27 1.00 -.16 -.29 -.13 _D_I§ -.48 .55 -. 16 1.00 -.02 -.10 _B_E_N -.34 .16 -.29 -.02 1.00 .38 REC .09 .08 -.13 -.10 .38 1.00 The uncorrected construct matrix from SPSS was used with the reliabilities for each construct provided by Program CFA. This is the equivalent 113 of correcting for attenuation. The correlation matrix from SPSS, corrected for attenuation by the reliabilities from Program CFA is more reflective of the exact structure of the data file. The correlations have been corrected for attenuation. To test the fit of the model. one has to assess the over-identified paths in the model. The over- identified paths are illustrated by the dotted grey arrows, and the identified paths are illustrated in the solid black arrows in Figure 12. The five non-constrained links were used to test that the errors in each of the non-constrained paths are equal to zero. Confidence intervals around the predicted correlations were used to assess the influence of sampling error. They are as follows: - 1-(-.20)2 / s/(317)=.054*(1.96)=.11 --> p(-.31 $-.20 $-.09)= .95 (SES/SID) - 1- (-.14)2 I s/(317)=.055*(1.96)=.11--> p(-.25 $-.14 $-.03)=.95(ACCIBEN) . 1- (-.38)2 I 7(317) =.048*(1.96)=.09--> p( -.47 $-.38$-.29)=.95 (ACC/DIS) - 1- (.08)2 I s/(317) =.056*(1.96)=.11 --> p( -.03 $.08 $.19)=.95 (DIS/BEN) - 1-(-.06)2 l\/(317) =.056*(1.96)=.11-—-> p( -.17 $-.06 $.05)=.95 (ACC/REC) The obtained correlations (see Table 17) for ACC/BEN. ACC/DIS, and ACC/REC are not within the confidence intervals. This indicates problems with the causal model as it has been conceived. Program Path’s local tests and global test for the analysis are as follows: Z value between SES and SID is -.64 (prob = .52); Z value between ACC and BEN is -2.22 (prob = .03); Z value between ACC and DIS is -1.17 (prob = .24); Z value between DIS and BEN is -1.08 (prob = .28); 2 value between ACC and REC is 1.49 (prob = .14); Chi square for overall fit is 10.98. df=5, p-value is .07. 114 Focusing on the above 2 scores and chi square, the model does fit the data, but there are problems with the model. The local test between ACC and BEN failed. This is evident because the z-value is less than -1.96, and the p- value is less than .05. In addition, the z-value of -1 .17 for the ACC/REC correlation is close to the 1.96 and -1.96 marks. Although considered significant by the z-test. these links failed the confidence interval test. The overall model fit the data, but there are some weak links. The probability for the overall model is greater than .05, and the obtained chi square is smaller than the critical chi square (df=5) of 11.07. Path analysis allows one to assess the errors and path coefficients, and consequently. respecify the path model. Observing the original error matrix, there appeared to be considerable problems with how ACC was related to DIS, BEN. and REC. There also was a large error with how DIS and BEN are related. These huge discrepancies indicate that perhaps these relationships should have been direct relationships to account for the large error. The researcher modified the paths to reflect direct relationships between each of the highlighted relationships in Table 18. 115 Table 18 Matrices for Original EPRP Model from Program Path Prflictfi correlations/Error Matrix‘ Path Coefficients ACC SID SES Dis BEN REc ACC SID SES DIS BEN REC ACC . -_, . Acc SID -.69 -.07 .oo .02 . SID -.69 SES .29 -.2o -.04 .01 . SES .29 .38 , Dis -.38 .55 -.12 . DIS .00 55 :61 BEN -.14 .14 -.28 .08 .02 BEN .00 .09 -26 00 REC -.06 .07 -.13-.06 .36 REC .00 .09 £53- 14 35 '-Error Matrix in Bold Because path coefficients indicate the direct paths, these coefficients should be faiiiy ample (greater than .10). The size of the direct paths for SES/DIS (-.01), and SES/REC (-.03), are relatively low coefficients when compared to the other direct paths. These direct paths were omitted in the next path model. The researcher decided to keep the two direct paths for SID/BEN (.09) and SID/REC (.09) to see how the path coefficients would be affected with the respecified model. A clearer picture of causality emerges as a result of the re-specification procedure. Thus. four additional direct correlations are introduced into the model, and two direct relationships are expunged from the model. One can see from the original correlation matrix that the direct correlations between ACC/DIS (-.48). ACC/BEN (-.34) are ample. However, the correlations between ACC/REC and DIS/BEN (-.02) are minor. 116 Keep in mind that correlations consist of direct. indirect, and spurious paths. If paths creating the correlation are both negative and positive, the combination could yield a small number. That is why the errors and path coefficients are assessed. The assessment is used as a basis to respecify how the model should have been entered into Program Path. Anderson and Gerbing cautioned the following: we recognize that most often some re-specification of the measurement model will be required. It must be stressed, however, that re-specification decisions should not be based on statistical considerations alone, but rather in conjunction with theory and content considerations. Consideration of theory and content both greatly reduces the number of alternate models to investigate and (cf. Young, 1977) reduces the possibility of taking advantage of sampling error to attain goodness of fit (1988, p. 416) The implication of this is that the reconceptualization (revision) has to make substantive (theoretical) sense as well as statistical sense. In summary, the proposed EPRP Model was consistent with the data. However, as a result of the individual link analysis, the evaluation of the error matrix, and path coefficients, it was found that the EPRP Model’s constructs are related to each other, but some of the hypothesized links were weak or hypothesized incorrectly. The path analysis allowed for a clearer picture to emerge. As such, a revised. theoretically consistent model involving the same constructs, and similar causality, was developed to test if the current insights hold We The next section discusses the insights which led to the revised model. 117 The Revised Model Substantively speaking, there is not much difference between the proposed EPRP Model (Figure 10) and the revised EPRP Model (Figure 12). They both hypothesize a similar array of relationships. The major difference is that in the initial EPRP Model, some relationships which were considered direct, were in fact indirect or spurious. and some relationships which were conceived as indirect were in fact direct. Figure 12 illustrates the revised version of the EPRP Model as a result of the initial path analysis. The major differences between the initially proposed model and the revised model are from three variables: ACC, SES, and SID. In the revised model, ACC has a direct effect on both BEN. DIS, and REC. This relationship was indirect and spurious in the original model. In addition, the causal arrows between SID/BEN and between SES/BEN were omitted. The revised model hypothesizes an indirect relationship between SES and REC, rather than direct, as originally hypothesized. Lastly. the revised model introduces a new relationship between DIS and BEN. They are hypothesized to be directly and spuriously related. 118 Figure 12. The EPRP Model (revised) ___ _ Parcaifllad __ _ _ \ Discrimination The revised model hypothesizes acculturation to be the direct antecedent. or driving force, behind the other constructs which are hypothesized in the literature as having an effect on recreation participation by ethnic group members. The extent to which one is acculturated has an impact on one’s socioeconomic standing, subcultural identity, perception of discrimination, and one’s perception of the benefits of parks, and actual recreation participation in the parks. The implications of this will be discussed later. This revised model must first be subjected to a path analysis to see if it is consistent with the data. Path Analysis for Revised Model The revised model has three non-constrained links to test. The following are the three correlations used to test the model: rsmes, rsems, and rsssmsc. The matrices given from Program Path is shown in Table 19. 119 Table 19 Matrices for EPRP Model from Prpgram Path (revised) Predicted correlatignlerror Matrix‘ Path Coefficients ACC SID SES DIS BEN REC ACC SiD SES DIS BEN REC ACC .oo .oo .oo .oo .00 ACC SiD -.69 -.07 .oo .02 .01 SID -.69 SES .29 -.2o -.02 .01 -.os SES .29 .oo DIS -.48 .55 -.14 .oo .oo DIS -.19 .43 .00 BEN -34 .14 -.3o -.03 .01 BEN --44 as --22 52.3 REC .09 .07 -.07 -.1o .37 REC -48 -36 .00 £05.38 ' - Error Matiix in Bold Considering the non-constrained paths, one tests the hypothesis that the errors in each of these paths are equal to zero (see error matrix in Table 19). Assessing the influence of sampling error, the confidence interval around the predicted correlations for the non-constrained paths are as follows: - 1-(-.20)2 l J(317)=.054*(1.96)=.11--> p(-.31 $-.20 $-.089) =.95(SES/SID) - 1- (-.14)’ I \/(317)=.055'(1.96)=.11—-> p(-.25 $-.14 $-.03)=.95(SESIDIS) - 1-(-.07)2 / ~/(317)=.056*(1.96)=.11-—-> p(-.18 $-.07 $.04) =.95 (SES/REC) All of the obtained correlations are within the confidence intervals of the predicted correlations. As a further test, the model was subjected to Program Path’s significance tests for the local tests and global test, they are as follows: Z value between SES and SID is -.64 (prob = .52); Z value between SES and DIS is -.17 (prob = .86); 2 value between SES and REC is -.55 (prob = .58); Chi square for overall fit is .74, df=3, p-value is .86. Note that all the z-values are greater than -1.96 and less than 1.96. In addition, the probabilities for the individual link analysis are all estimated above 120 .05. The overall chi square is very much smaller than the chi square for the initial model (.74 versus 10.09) with a p-value of .86 (as compared to .07 for the initial model). Both the local and global tests, as well as the confidence interval tests confirm that the model is consistent with the data, and the model is not rejected. The hypothesis that the data are consistent with the model is supported. If the model fits, then all correlations are significant. Although significant, the enor matrix and the path coefficient matrix were assessed to discern if the model could be made stronger (see Table 19). Rudimentary inspection of the error matrix reveals that the errors are small. However. the path coefficient matrix indicates two relatively low coefficients for SID/BEN (-.08) and DIS/REC (-.05). These path coefficients should be substantial (relative to the other path coefficients) because they are direct links and the errors are not different from zero. This indicates that perhaps the relationships are indirect, rather than direct. One final path analysis was performed on the data. The Final Revised Model The difference between Figure 12 and Figure 13 is that there is no direct arrow from SID to BEN, and no direct arrow from DIS to REC. Substantively what this means is that the previous direct relationships are now indirect. Perceived discrimination has a direct effect on the perceptions of benefits of public recreation rather than on actual participation. Actual participation is influenced by discrimination via discrimination’s effect on perceived benefits. In 121 addition, subcultural identity does not affect perceived benefits directly. It is re- conceived as influencing perceived benefits with discrimination as a filter. Figure 13. The EPRP Model (final version) Discrimination Path Analysis for Fi_n_al Revised EPRP Model The final revised model has five non-constrained links to test. The following are the five correlations used to test the model: rsmss. rswms, ram“. rsemc’ and rmc. The matrices given from Program Path are shown in Table 20. The confidence intervals around the predicted correlations are as follows: - 1-(-.20)2 I J(317)=.054 *(1.96)=.11—-> p(-.31 $-.20 $-.09) =.95(SESISID) . 1- (—.14)2 I ~/(317)=.055 *(1.96)=.11--> p(-.25 $-.14 $-.03)=.95(SES/DIS) - 1-(-.18)2 / ~/(317)=.054 *(1.96)=.11-—-> p(.07 $-.18 $.29) =.95 (SID/BEN) - 1-(-.07)2 I J(317)=.056 *(1.96)=.11—-> p(-.18 $-.07 $.04) =.95 (SES/REC) - 1-(-.06)2 I t/(317)=.056 *(1.96)=.11—-> p(-.17 $-.06 $.05) =.95(DISIREC) All of the obtained correlations were within the confidence interval. The fit of the model was also assessed using the local and global tests. The z-values and chi square are as follows: 122 Z value between SES and SID is -.64 (prob = .52); Z value between SES and DIS is -.17 E (prob = .86); 2 value between SID and BEN is -.19 (prob = .85); Z value between SES and REC is -.55 (prob = .58); Z value between DIS and REC is -.30 (prob = .76); Chi square for overall fit is .87, df=5, p-value is .97. Table 20 Matrices for EPRP Model from Prggram Path (final version) 2mm correlationsfiror Matfl‘ Bath Coefficients ACC SID SES DIS BEN REC ACC SID SES DIS BEN REC ACC 1.00 .00 .oo .oo .oo .00 ACC .oo SID -.69 1.00 -.07 .oo -.02 -.01 SID -.69 .oo SES .29 -.201.00 -.02 .oo -.01 SES .29 .00 .00 DIS -.48 .55 -.141.00 .00 -.03 DIS -.19 .43 .00 .00 BEN -.34 .18 -.29 -.03 1.00 -.01 BEN -.40 -00 521 =25 -00 REC .09 .09 ..07 -.06 .39 1_oo REC .50 .34 .00 .00 .49 .00 " - Error Matrix in Bold All of the z-values are near zero, and have p-values well over .05. In addition, the overall chi square was .87. This chi square was slightly higher than the first revised model’s chi square (.74). This is due to five degrees of freedom (versus the three degrees of freedom in the first revised model). The p-value is higher than the first revised model. This is indicative of less error and therefore a stronger model. All of the path coefficients are substantial, and the errors are within sampling error of zero. The model is consistent with the data. The final model is the best possible model given substantive and empirical measures. Figure 14 illustrates the final model with the corresponding path coefficients. A comparison of all three models and their respective errors, z-values, and p- values is included in Appendix D. 123 Interpretation of the Model Figure 14 represents the path coefficients in the model. The reader should keep in mind that in a standardized linear one-predictor case, the parameter estimate or path coefficient (beta weight) is the correlation. The indirect and spurious paths, however. also have to be taken into account when assessing the full impact that one construct can have on another. As an example, the direct relationship between ACC and DIS is -.19. However, DIS has an indirect relationship between ACC via SID of (-.69*.43) = -.29. The total effect of ACC on DIS is (-.19 + -.29) -.48. Figure 14. The EPRP Model (final version) of *- ~-19 PM 3 6 Discnmrnation S bcult I “1|;an um ] .34 This reflects both the direct and indirect effects that ACC has on DIS. If one observes the obtained correlation matrix, one notices that -.48 is the correlation between ACC and DIS (see Table 19). The implication of this is that 40% (-.19/-.48) of the relationship between ACC and DIS is accounted for by the 124 direct relationship, and 60% is accounted for by the indirect relationship with SID. This process (statistical procedure) of path analysis can be considered as a series of regressions in which each variable in the model is considered in turn as a dependent variable to be predicted from its causal antecedent variables. The path coefficients are equal to standardized regression coefficients (beta weights). Therefore, the relative influence one variable (construct) has over another variable in the model can be compared. Having mentioned that, the hypotheses can be examined. Examination of Hypotheses Hypotheses 1. Subcultural identity [SID] and socioeconomic status [SES] are significant predictors of public recreation participation [REC]. Subcultural identity will be a stronger predictor of park use than socioeconomic status. There is a significant relationship between perceived discrimination [DIS] and public recreation participation, and perceived benefits of recreation [BEN] and public recreation participation. There is a significant relationship between the subcultural identity factor [SID] and socioeconomic status [SES]. There is a significant negative relationship between acculturation [ACC] and subcultural identity. There is a significant positive relationship between acculturation and socioeconomic status. There is a positive relationship between subcultural identity and perceived discrimination. There is a negative relationship between perceived discrimination [DIS] and public recreation participation [REC]. The path model hypothesis: 125 Ho: the data are consistent with the model (Error = 0). H,: the data are inconsistent with the model (Error :0). Subcultural identity and socioeconomic status on glblic recreation usage. SID and SES are significant predictors of park use (REC). Subcultural identity was hypothesized as a stronger predictor of park use than socioeconomic status. Looking at Figure 14, one can see that the correlation for SES/REC via BEN is (-.21 *.49) -.10, and the correlation for SID/REC is the correlation via DIS and BEN and the direct relationship between SID and REC: [(.43 *-.25 * .49)+(.34)] = -.29. The hypotheses are supported. Subcultural identity and socioeconomic status are significant predictors, and subcultural identity is the stronger predictor. Perceived discrimination and perceived benefits on park use. There is a significant relationship between perceived discrimination and park use, and perceived benefits of parks and park use. The relationship is not as originally hypothesized in terms of its causality, but nonetheless the relationships between DIS and park use, and BEN and park use are significant. As Figure 14 indicates, BEN is a significant cause of park use with a .49 effect. Perceived discrimination has a lesser (-.12), but still significant effect on recreation participation. There is a negative relationship between the perceived discrimination and park use, and a positive relationship between perceived benefits and park use. These correlations support the hypotheses that the relationship between perceived discrimination and park use is negative, and the correlation between perceived benefits and park use is positive. 126 Sufiultugl ident'y and socioeconomic status relationship. There is a significant relationship between the subcultural identity factor and socioeconomic status. The path model would support this hypothesis. The correlation between SID and SES is (-.69 * .29) -.20. This finding offers support for the marginality and ethnicity theories. According to this model, they are spuriously related. From a substantive view, the relevance of this is that, in previous studies, they were often viewed as separate or dissimilar to warrant different studies or approaches. There is a strong relationship between the two, especially given the antecedent of acculturation as hypothesized by the researcher. Acculturation/subcultural identity and acculturation/socioeconomic status. There is a significant negative relationship between ACC and SID, and there is a significant positive relationship between ACC and SES. The model clearly presents the highest direct (and negative) correlation between ACC and SID as -.69. ACC also has a strong positive relationship (.29) with SES. These hypotheses are supported and acculturation has a significant impact on socioeconomic status and subcultural identity. Subcultural identity and perceived discrimination. There is a positive relationship between subcultural identity and perceived discrimination. The path model reveals a .43 correlation between SD and DIS. The hypothesis that subcultural identity positively influences perceived discrimination is supported. 127 mm Fin i This summary is organized in a fashion consistent with path analysis, whereby one discusses the individual links, and then provides an overall assessment. The researcher first discusses the individual constructs and the relationships among those constructs. This provides a comparison to results of other studies. After the constructs are discussed, the researcher provides a summary of the overall model, and discusses how to de—construct the model to compare it to other studies. A ti n rceiv nefits and erceived discrimination. Acculturation, as conceived in this study, reflects a preference by Puerto Ricans to maintain the use of Spanish as the primary mode of communication. The assumption is that this indicates a preference to maintain the heterogeneity of a Puerto Rican subculture within the US. supra culture. As such, it indicates the extent to which a Puerto Rican, as a member of an ethnic group, is assimilated into the mainstream. The indicators were similar to those used in Floyd, et al.’s (1993) study. The current study supported Floyd, et al.’s idea that acculturation (cultural distance) plays a critical role in the relationship between one’s subcultural identity (intergroup distance) and socioeconomic status (socioeconomic distance). Although the concepts were operationalized differently, they nonetheless held true. Acculturation was found to be an important causal antecedent to several variables in the EPRP Model. 128 A major finding regarding acculturation was the strong direct effect on perception of park benefits (-.40) and perceived discrimination (-.19). Perceived discrimination was found to negatively affect public recreation, as expected. However, the direct relationship was not as robust as the other direct relationships. The path analysis revealed the relationship to be indirect. Perceived benefits of the recreation environment have not been explicitly looked at in previous studies. Often times, it is assumed that there are perceptions of parks, but those perceptions are often not measured because it is a "given.” The model illustrates the importance of measuring perceived benefits because it is a mediating variable between three different causal processes. The negative relationship between acculturation and perceived discrimination supports a marriage between West's (1989) discrimination theory and Floyd, et al.’s (1993) acculturation theory. The measurement of values was initially incorporated in this study as suggested by Washbume (1978) and Hutchison (1987). It differed from Hutchison’s model (see Figure 6) in that Hutchison conceived of ethnicity/race as an antecedent to values, whereas, this study argues the opposite. Cortes, et al. (1994) specifically argued for language and value items in the formation of a unidimensional acculturation scale. This study did not support that argument. The value and language items, some of which were taken from Cortes, et al.’s study, did not pass the internal consistency test for unidimensionality. As such, items measuring values were left out of the analysis and not incorporated into the acculturation construct. 129 Socipflnomic status and subcultural identity. These two constructs reflect the marginality and ethnicity theories, respectively. The inclusion of both constructs is consistent with the recent Iiterature’s embracement of both theories. Washbume (1978) mentioned that reasons for underparticipation were not simply due to SES, and forecasted declining utility for SES. While this study supports the notion that participation is not due solely to SES, the study does not support declining utility for SES. This study supported the idea of an interaction effect between SES and SID, initially considered by Hutchison (1987). According to the model, the relationship between SES and SID is spurious. As hypothesized, and in support of Stamps and Stamps (1985) and Washbume, ethnicity was found to be a more important predictor than marginality. In addition, it was found that both SES and SID’s effect on using public parks was minimal compared to the effect of other factors in the model. SID was operationalized a little differently than in Taylor's (1992) study. She looked at ethnic expression of leisure activities and inferred SID from this. This study took a more subjective and direct approach and solicited information reflecting ethnic expression (see questions measuring SID, Appendix C). The SID measure was also more robust than West’s (1989) measure of subculture, and was similar to Floyd, et al.’s (1993) conception. West found no support for the relationship between subcultural identity and recreation in his study. This study did find support for the relationship, albeit a spurious one. 130 Socioeconomic status was measured using an adopted version of the Hollingshead Two-Factor Index of education and occupation which was used in Stamps and Stamps’ (1985) study. This differed from Floyd, et al.’s study which only used education as an indicator of SES. Unlike Stamps and Stamps, this study did not collapse the sample into upper and lower classes, but instead maintained the two indicators as reflective of one construct. Public reereetion participation. Unlike previous studies that observed what people do at parks, or asked about which recreation activities were participated in, this study focused on frequency of visits to parks over a one year time period. Park visitation was used as a proxy for public recreation participation. The interest in this study was in park use as measured by visitation. The advantage of measuring park use in this manner is that one can procure users and non-users of the parks. Not all parks were used because they did not meet the internal consistency criterion for unidimensionality of the public recreation participation construct. Chapter Summeg The EPRP Model was submitted to stringent validity tests of internal consistency and parallelism. The constructs held and were used as summative variables in a path analysis where the model was then analyzed to estimate the parameters and test for the fit of the model. The EPRP Model’s initial conception in terms of direct or indirect causality was respecified after assessing path coefficients, errors, and confidence intervals. The result was a revised model (Figure 14) which was statistically 131 consistent with the data, and substantively consistent with the theoretical underpinnings of this study. The result of this section was the testing of what became a sound theoretical and statistical model: The Ethnicity and Public Recreation Participation Model. The next, and final chapter summarizes the major points of this study, discusses the deconstruction of the EPRP model, and considers the implications for future studies. 132 Chapter 5 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The purpose of this study was to identify factors which were important in examining the relationship between ethnicity and recreation behavior. This was accomplished in one of two ways. The first approach was to create a theoretical model that underscored the relationships between the different factors as hypothesized in the literature regarding ethnicity/race and recreation. The researcher identified six factors: (1) ethnicity (subcultural identity), (2) marginality (socioeconomic status), (3) acculturation, (4) perceived benefits of public recreation, (5) perceived discrimination, and (6) public recreation participation. Based on prior empirical studies, the six factors were assembled to provide the building blocks for the Ethnicity and Public Recreation Participation Model. This led to the second approach in which the factors were examined. The theoretical model was subjected to a confirmatory factor analysis to validate the existence of the factors/constnrcts as measured by the items that made up each construct. Once validity was assessed, the model was then empirically subjected to a path analysis in order to estimate the parameters of the EPRP Model, and test the fit of the model by assessing whether or not the model was consistent with the data. The researcher revised the model based on the path coefficient and error analysis and found that the revised model was not different, conceptually, from the initial model. As a result, this study provides a theoretically and empirically 133 sound model to consider in reconceptualizing the relationships between ethnicity and marginality factors and their effect on public recreation participation. mm Dflpnstructing the EPRP Model: Reflections of previous literature. The purpose of this section is to provide the reader with an in—depth look at the nuances of the EPRP Model. Because the model is reflective of causality, and it is statistically and substantively sound, all or parts of the model should provide points of comparison to other studies. The researcher de-constructed the model to illustrate, pedagogically, how the model reflects prior literature and reconceptualizes the relationships. One method of approaching this de- construction is to discern what would happen with the removal of one or several parts of the model. The two key constructs are acculturation and perceived benefits. The former because it is a significant driving force in the model. The latter because it is both central to the model, and it is a ”new" constmct advocated for and introduced in this study. The removal of a construct does not change the correlation between the constructs, rather the changes occur in the path coefficients, i.e., in the nature of the direct relationships. Rempvel pf ammatiop. The EPRP Model incorporates theories espoused specifically by Floyd, et al. (1993), Washbume (1978) and West (1985). These are acculturation, marginality and ethnicity, and discrimination, respectively. If one removes Floyd, et al.’s contribution, the acculturation construct as a direct driving force is extracted. The resulting path model is 134 exhibited in Figure 15. The curved arrow indicates that SID and SES are spuriously related, but that one does not hypothesize a common antecedent. If one compares Figure 15 with Figure 14, one sees that the path coefficients have changed. Figure 15. The EPRP Model without Acculturation {Mama ' ° m tatus - Recreation Ix 0. -.27 ' Perceived Discrimination Subcultural [Identity ,, ,, .02 In causal modeling, if any variable is thrown out of a model, it reduces to a simpler model. The implication of this is that if the larger model fits, then the resulting simple model or causal strings will also fit. The model in Figure 15 has a chi square of 2.05, with df=4, and a p-value of .73. Acculturation as a driving force behind DIS and SID increases the effect that each of those constructs have on BEN and REC, respectively. The inclusion of ACC in the model has the effect of lessening the effect SID and SES will have on other variables (and themselves). A general rule of causal analysis 135 is that correlations are going to be progressively smaller as variables are farther away from each other in the causal string. The implication of this is that without consideration of ACC as the driving force behind DIS and SID, the effect of DIS on BEN decreases to a coefficient of -.07 (versus -.25 with ACC in the model). Unlike, the final EPRP Model, the model without acculturation (Figure 15) would show a stronger direct link between DIS and REC (-.10) than between DIS and BEN. Figure 15 reconceptualizes and combines the relationships in Figure 4 (Washbume) and Figure 7 (West). Removal of perceived benefite. Figure 16 reflects the combined concepts of Figure 4 (Washbume), Figure 7 (West) and Figure 9 (Floyd, et al.). The model has a chi square of .44, with df=2, and a p-value of .81. The following is a comparison between the path coefficients in the full EPRP Model versus the model without BEN in it: Qaueal Path Model with BEN Model without BEN ACC/SID -.69 -.69 ACC/SES .29 .29 ACC/DIS -.19 -.19 ACC/REC .50 .28 SID/REC .34 .31 DIS/REC --- -.16 SES/REC ---- .09 The comparison of the path coefficients is instructive for several reasons. First, one can see that paths that were not affected by BEN were unchanged, as 136 the model should predict. Second, because the direct relationship between ACC and BEN was eliminated (-.40), it attenuated the relationship between ACC and REC. Third, because BEN is not in the model, the relationship between DIS/REC is direct and it changes the relationship between SID/REC slightly. Related to this, the relationship between DIS/REC and SES/REC were not defined as direct in the EPRP Model, but they are when the BEN mediator is removed in order to be consistent with causality. Lastly, this is the first model Flgure 16. The EPRP Model without Perceived Recreation Benefits that presents a positive correlation between SES/REC. All prior models have shown the relationship to be negative. Removal of acculturation and perceived benefits. If ACC and BEN were removed from the model, the result would be a model reflecting the three underlying theories of marginality, ethnicity, and discrimination (see Figure 17). 137 It comes closest to Stamps and Stamps (1985) model (see Figure 5), and unites it with West’s (1989) model (see Figure 7). This is the ”basic” model as conceived today by most of the researchers studying ethnicity and recreation. Figure 17. The EPRP Model without ACC and BEN -.27 Participation Perceived _ Discrimination '20 When comparing path coefficients of Figure 17 with those of Figure 14, the importance of the role of discrimination is noted it ACC and BEN are not accounted for. The strong negative path coefficient (-.20) is the strongest it has been in any of the models. This affirms West’s claim that discrimination is a valid construct when looking at ethnicity and recreation. Without ACC driving the model, or perceived benefits mediating relationships, the relative impacts of SES and SID on REC are about the same. This provides insight into why studies have been ambiguous in the past as to which of the two is the stronger predictor of recreation participation. The coefficient of .15 between SiD/REC does not support the notion that minorities are under-represented in public parks 138 because of ethnicity. This relationship is mediated by other factors as was illustrated in the previous models. The result of this deconstruction is that perceived benefits and acculturation are both important constructs to look at when considering ethnicity and recreation. Through causal modeling, the relative impacts of the inclusion or removal of a construct can be seen and compared. Conclusions Based on the findings, and within the limitations of this study, the researcher felt that this study answered the call from Floyd (1998) and Henderson (1998) for a more integrative approach, and more complex measurement of the relationships between ethnicity and recreation. The more complex measurement came in the form of identifying critical dimensions from the literature and empirically testing these dimensions through confirmatory and path analytic approaches. This study found the relationships in the model to be consistent with those hypothesized by the literature, although the nature of the relationships are reoonceptualized. From a conceptual standpoint, subcultural identity and acculturation were reoonceptualized: the former in terms of how it is operationalized, the latter in terms of how it related to other variables. Subcultural identity was reoonceptualized as a subjective response to the extent to which one identifies with a subcultural group. This is different from previous studies that simply ask or denote (in observational studies) whether one is Black, Caucasian, or Hispanic, and is consistent with Washbume’s (1978) initial conception. 139 Acculturation was reoonceptualized as a precursor to the entire model. In previous studies it was conceptualized as occurring simultaneously with socioeconomic status or subcultural identity. The study answered the first research question regarding which of the two factors (ethnicity or marginality) was the better predictor of recreation behavior. In terms of their direct effect, ethnicity has a more profound effect. The second research question considered the individual and combined importance of ethnicity and marginality factors. The combined effect, as measured by the SES and SID constructs, was found to be significant (see findings). The last research question considered the relationship between acculturation, perceived discrimination, and perceived benefits. initially, it was thought that the relationship was indirect via subcultural identity and socioeconomic status, respectively. The path analysis of the EPRP Model, however, supported the idea that there were considerable direct effects from acculturation to both perceived benefits and perceived discrimination. The EPRP Model not only determined the relative influence of the factors in the model (see Figure 14), but also offered a "snap shot" of the relationships between the most prominent theories regarding ethnicity and recreation. As a result, the EPRP Model contributes to the literature by offering a model for practitioners and researchers to consider. The model is not exhaustive of, but rather, instructive of possible factors influencing ethnic participation in public recreation settings. 140 Shortcomings and Limitations There are some shortcomings and limitations with respect to the model. From a procedural standpoint, the study had to alter the original use of the SES scale. Because individual income was not used, the household income was not appropriate to use where education and occupation were individual measures. Although the concepts are based on previous studies, variables were operationalized differently. For example, subcultural identity in the current study is operationalized as the extent to which a person identifies with his/her subculture. In previous studies, it was a categorical variable asking for the person to “check” his/her ethnic/racial background. Because variables were operationalized differently, the reader should be careful when comparing findings to other studies. The dependent variable, REC, was limited to frequency of park visits. i does not consider purpose of visit or the activities engaged in. Furthermore, it was limited to three parks instead of five. Several factors could explain this. Possibilities include the location of Morris Street Park or the heavy use by students at McMahon Field. it may also be because of non-response bias. If only 27% of the eligible Puerto Rican population was surveyed, the other 73% could have a different view of those parks. Whatever the reasons, the construct, includes 60% of the public parks in Southbridge. But, of the parks that did remain in the construct, they are the closest or most heavily used parks by Puerto Ricans in Southbridge. 141 Related to REC is the perceived recreation benefits constnrct. BEN measures whether respondents perceive that, in general, they have benefits versus what specifically those benefits are. As such, the BEN construct is a measure was intended to be a measure of overall perceptions, versus individual motivations for visiting a park. The acculturation constnrct presents another limitation. ACC appeared to be very central to the causality of the model. This may have been due to the population under study. Puerto Ricans, in general, have a particular historical relationship with the United States that is beyond the scope of this study. Southbridge Puerto Ricans, in particular, have a very strong tie to Puerto Rico. This may influence the acculturation process. Lastly, the study population is from a specific ethnic group. Although a Latino group, these findings should not be generalized to all Latinos. The findings should also not be generalized to other ethnic group members. Some of the items which reflect the constructs are specific to Puerto Ricans. Acculturation, subcultural identity, and discrimination measures should reflect the specific ethnic group under study. lmplicatione Congptuel implications. From a conceptual standpoint, the model synthesizes past research and looks at the relationship between the different factors. Marginality and ethnicity theories as explanations are insufficient, and do not operate in a vacuum. They are affected by each other and other factors 142 in direct, indirect and spurious ways. The challenge is to try and make sense out of what these relationships really mean. This model also helps to fill a void in theory-building approaches to studying ethnicity and recreation constnrcts. In particular, the EPRP Model introduces the perceived benefits of park use/recreation as a major factor intervening or mediating the relationships between SES, ACC, and SID on recreation participation. This incorporates leisure theory into the model, a theory which had not been directly incorporated in prior studies. This study looks at affective or behavioral traits, rather than objective manifestations. In addition, the perspective or point of reference is from the actual ethnic group, and therefore the norm is the Puerto Rican norm. In previous studies, subcultural group members are often compared to whites. This assumes a normative structure based on white’s recreation. If one is to tnrly understand the nature of ethnicity with respect to recreation, one should study ethnic groups without using Whites as a standard. The comparison to Whites or other subcultural groups brings in the pedagogical issue of race versus ethnicity. This study circumvented the issue by not considering it at all. As stated eariier, the issue is one of perspective. Practical impligtions for the fielg. The last conceptual implication relates to a practical one. This study illustrates that Puerto Ricans do use parks. Some more heavily than others. To continue to view ethnic group members as being ”under-represented” in a recreation site is to assume a normative order based 143 on Whites. From a demographic standpoint, when the minority populations become the majority, this will no longer be an adequate point of reference. The model shows that asking minorities about how they perceive the resource is important to understanding why they visit a recreation site. This is a better approach than observing that there are not as many Blacks as Whites visiting the park. If one understands the perceptions or motivations behind the visit, one could better cater the product to the user. This is implicit in most studies, but it should be made more explicit. The understanding that acculturation is a significant driving force has several ramifications for practitioners. It indicates that the sense of belonging one has to American society impacts public recreation. If one does not feel accepted in the United States, one is not likely to participate in public places, regardless of subcultural identity. One can have a strong subcultural identity, but if one feels helshe belongs, one will still participate in the public sphere. The implication of this is that parks should be promoted as cultural centers, and recreation programs need to be more inclusive. It is worthwhile to emphasize the importance of understanding the different leisure patterns and needs of various ethnic groups. If acculturation does indeed drive several of the other factors influencing public participation in recreation areas, practitioners need to take the time to know more about the background of subcultural groups using the resource. In Southbridge, for example, a majority of the respondents preferred the survey in Spanish and/or preferred to speak Spanish. This knowledge should be 144 incorporated into the management of the resource. For example, if there are rules which regulate use of the parks, and the majority of ones users are non- English speakers, then managers need to ensure that the signage is bilingual. Cultural nuances are important to keep in mind when trying to enforce policies. Because acculturation affects ones perception of discrimination, managers can do one of two things. Managers and employees can be more sensitive to this issue. By acknowledging that there is already sufficient discrimination outside of the park, recreation managers and staff should work harder to provide a welcoming atmosphere, with no tolerance for discriminatory practices. Related to this, managers should hire a diverse group of employees who are members of various ethnic groups and who can ”speak their language.” Policies today need to reflect the users of tomonow. A more proactive, assertive stance on reaching out to minority groups is recommended. In the case of the Latino population, studies show that they are very brand loyal (Wagner 8. Soberon-Ferrer, 1990). Starting a loyal following, based on cultural understanding today, will ensure visitation in the more pluralistic environment of the future. For example, Henry Street Park was visited more than any other park in this study. Administrators of the park realized that the majority of the users were Puerto Rican, and that the majority spoke Spanish. Nearly a decade ago, tennis courts were changed to reflect the pattern of use of the Puerto Ricans who preferred to play basketball instead of tennis, and signage is bilingual. These small but significant changes did not go unnoticed by the Puerto Rican 145 population. Over the years, Henry Street Park has hosted several Puerto Rican festivals. As a result, perceptions of discrimination were lessened and the perception of the park changed, thereby leading to more use. As a recreation professional in a public service agency, exploration of reasons for why people do not visit a recreation area are sought. Often times, because the area is public, and the economic impact is minimal, minorities in the public sector constituency are overlooked. A needs assessment should go beyond recreation needs, and incorporate the cultural needs of the community. Recommendations for Further Study Procedural recommendations. If the study were to be conducted again, more time should be allotted to the distribution and collection of the survey. Related to this, third class mailing should be avoided if possible. Most of the problems with responses and follow-ups resulted from problems with third class mailing. Specifically, there were problems with the time that it took for the Southbridge Post Office to distribute third class mail to the population because it was not a priority. Less of a recommendation, and more of a reminder, the researcher stresses the importance of making contact with several leaders in the community. Time has to be given for the contacts to establish credibility for the researcher and the study. Questipnnaire recommendations. In order to better reflect the Hollingshead Index, response categories should have an option for the spouse so that total household income could be include. In addition, the employment 146 status of all working-age household members, and their occupations, should be asked. More items are needed to capture the essence of each of the different construct areas. Given more space on the survey, the researcher would have included more items to measure the constructs. In general, the more items one has the better the reliability of the scales. Specifically, more items measuring subcultural identity and socioeconomic status would have strengthened those scales. In the demographic section of the study inclusion of both sets of grandparents should have been included (see Appendix A). If one looks at section 7B1-7B5, it is clear that the second set of grandparents was not asked about. This was an oversight on the part of the researcher. In order to use the information, it would have to be assumed that when the respondent answered, they were referring to both sets of grandparents. This is not an unreasonable assumption given that most of the respondents are emigrants or of the first generation in Southbridge. Statistical reggmmendations. Generally, there has been little use of confirmatory factor analysis and path analysis techniques in the recreation literature. As the technique has demonstrated, causal models are very unforgiving of error. The validity check is very rigid, and the reliability of ones measures is incorporated into the measurement model. The process also allows one to control for statistical artifacts such as measurement error. 147 As a result, it causes the researcher to be more precise in his or her theorizing and in the tests of those theories. Diagnosing errors is a skill that is sharpened with experience. The statistical techniques used in this study were very useful diagnostic tools in assessing where the model went wrong, or where the researchers thinking was incorrect, and where the model could be strengthened. These statistical techniques are exceptional for theory development and testing. @ngptual recommendations. As mentioned earlier, the model is not exhaustive, but there are certain variables from the study which could be explored in future studies. Age, for example, might moderate the effect of acculturation or subcultural identity on one’s perception of discrimination. Age might also have a bigger impact. Location was another common factor which was mentioned in the literature but not assessed in the current model. It is recommended that a spatial analysis be used as a means to determining if place of residence (determined by physical location) has an influence on (specific) park use. It would be useful to compare whether location or subcultural identity is a better predictor of which park one would use. Generational influence was not used in this study for reasons noted in the questionnaire recommendations. However, in a future study, generational status could be looked at to see what effect it has on acculturation. Conceptually, it is conceived as a precursor to acculturation so that level of acculturation is affected by generational status. It could be hypothesized that as generational 148 status increases (O=emigrant, 1=first generation, etc), the level of acculturation (into the mainstream) increases. Lastly, the study needs to be replicated. Specifically, the study should be replicated among other ethnic groups. Use the same constructs, but alter the substantive nature of the itemsfrndicators to reflect the particular ethnic group under study. The concepts would still have to be subjected to a confirmatory factor analysis. The purpose would be to see if the model holds true for other ethnic groups. Summagy This study makes several contributions to the existing knowledge regarding ethnicity and recreation behavior. First, it provides support for the ethnicity, marginality, discrimination, and acculturation theories. Second, it provides support for the inclusion of leisure variables when assessing recreation behavior - these are variables often overlooked. Third, this study found that the acculturation process is a significant factor. It is hypothesized to be the antecedent of all the other factors. Fourth, this study uses statistical techniques not utilized in prior research on this topic. Fifth, the sample collection technique could be used as a model for other difficult to reach ethnic groups. Finally, this study provides a model for the reconceptualization of the relationship between ethnicity, marginality, and related factors, and their effect on public recreation participation. 149 APPENDIX A English and Spanish Questionnaires with Descriptive Statistics 150 Southbridge The Department 'ecreation, and Touri;" rces Michigan sity East La “~61; ‘ t u A 2 151 INSTRUCTIONS The survey is meant to capture the opinions of the Puerto Rican community in Southbridge, Massachusetts. The purpose of this study is to better understand how Puerto Ricans interact in public recreation settings. The person whose name appears on the envelope should be the 'one responding to the questionnaire. It is important that you answer the following questions as honestly as possible so that the study can accurately represent the Puerto Rican community. Please take your time answering the questions. The questionnaire should take from 10-12 minutes to complete. When you are finished, fold the questionnaire once and place it in the yellow envelope provided for you. You do not need to put a stamp on the envelope, simply drop it in the mail. If you received two questionnaires (one in English, the other in Spanish), please return only one completed questionnaire. Thank you! Any questions or concerns please contact: Edwin Gomez, Doctoral Candidate Department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources 131 Natural Resources Building Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824-1222 Phone: (517) 353-9145 E-mail: gomczedw@pilotmsu.edu 152 Page 1. — SECTION ONE: PARK QUESTIONS 1. People have suggested many reasons why they visit parks. Below is a list of the benefits of going to a park. Please indicate whether you strongly agree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree. Neither rec Strongly or Strongly Agree Agree Disagree Disagree Disagree "/ °/ ‘V °/ "/ A. A benefit oifioing to Southbridge parks ( o) ( o) ( o) ( o) ( o) is that they ow me to enjoy nature. 38.2 39.5 14.4 6.0 1.8 B. A benefit of oing to Southbri c parks is that they low me to escape or a while. 39.0 43.5 10.7 4.2 2.6 C. A benefit of goin to Southbridge parks is they allow me to socifiizdcreate personal contacts. 29.1 48.9 12.8 6.4 2.7 D. A benefit of going to Southbridge parks is that they allow me to get some exercise. 37.4 42.9 11.8 6.6 1.3 E. A benefit of going to Southbrid e arks is that they allow me to spend time wi amily/friends. 50.4 37.6 6.1 4.5 1.1 F. A benefit of having parks in Southbridge is that they offer a place with lots of open space. 38.1 43.6 10.0 6.6 1.8 G. A benefit of having parks in Southbridge is that they ofi'er a place for children and youth to go. 61.5 29.8 3.4 3.7 1.6 2. How 0 en during each season do you use or visit each of the following public parks in So ridge? A. MORRIS STREET PARK (Almost Never) (Sometimes) (Frequent) (Very Frequent) (A Lot) Never Once 2-4 times 5-13 times 14-20 times 21-29 times 30+ times (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) Summer (June-August) 43.8 1 1.7 18.2 17.7 4.2 1.8 2.6 Fall (September-November) 70.3 7.6 14.8 4.4 2.3 0.5 0.0 Winter (December-February) 85.9 4.4 7.0 1.8 0.8 0.0 0.0 Spring (March-May) 63.0 6.8 14.3 8.9 2.9 1.9 0.5 B. WEST STREET PARK (Almost Never) (Sometimes) (Frequent) (Very Frequent) (A Lot) Never Once 2-4 times 5-13 times 14-20 times 21-29 times 30+ times (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) Summer (June-August) 42.3 12.3 21.9 16.4 3.9 0.8 2.3 Fall (September-November) 71.4 7.6 10.7 7.3 2.1 0.5 0.5 Winter (December-February) 89.6 2.6 5.7 0.0 0.5 0.0 0.0 Spring (March-May) 64.1 6.8 15.4 9.1 2.1 2.1 0.5 153 Page 2. C. HENRY STREET PARK (Almost Never) (Sometimes) (Frequent) (Very Frequent) (A Lot) Never Once 2-4 times 5-13 times 14-20 times 21-29 times 30+ times (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) Summer (June-August) 19.3 8.6 19.3 16.7 12.0 7.0 17.2 Fall (September-November) 50.0 8.1 19.5 12.0 4.7 2.3 3.4 Winter (December-February) 79.1 5.2 9.1 3.9 1.6 0.5 0.5 Spring (March-May) 41.9 8.3 18.5 11.2 10.2 3.6 6.3 D. WESTVILLE DAM RECREATION AREA (Airnost Never) (Sometimes) (Frequent) (Very Frequent) (A Lot) Never Once 2-4 times 5—13 times 14-20 times 21-29 times 30+ times (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) Summer (June-August) 8.1 9.9 24.7 29.4 14.8 4.9 8.1 Fall (September-November) 51.3 12.2 22.9 7.3 3.4 1.8 1.0 Winter (December-February) 82.3 8.1 6.5 1.8 1.0 0.3 0.0 Spring (March-May) 40.1 9.1 23.7 15.6 7.3 2.3 1 8 E. HARRY J. McMAHON MEMORIAL FIELD (Dresser St.) (Almost Never) (Sometimes) (Frequent) (Very Frequent) (A Lot) Never Once 24 times 5-13 times 14-20 times 21-29 times 30+ times (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) Summer (June-August) 46.1 10.4 14.3 12.8 8.6 2.1 5.7 Fall (September-November) 62.8 8.9 10.9 7.0 4.9 2.1 3.4 Winter (December-February) 89.1 4.4 2.9 0.8 1.6 0.8 0.5 Spring (March-May) 62.2 6.5 11.7 8.3 6.0 2.1 3.1 SECTION TWO: PUERTO RICAN CULTURE QUESTIONS 3. Please indicate which language(s) you prefer to use. More More S anish Use En sh Only an Both an Onl Spanish English Equally Spanish Eng sh (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) A. Preferred language at home 3.9 14.1 29.2 34.4 18.5 B. Preferred language for radio and television. 12.1 21.8 34.9 19.4 11.8 C. Preferred language for magazines/newspapers. 22.2 16.2 25.3 16.4 19.8 D. Preferred language for music. 5.3 10.8 35.1 25.3 23.5 E. Preferred language your close friends speak. 9.2 13.1 30.2 28.1 19.4 F. Preferred language at parties and get-to others. 6 1 12.7 34.2 28.6 18.3 G. Preferred language when you visit poop e or 4 8 12.1 34.4 31.2 17.5 when people visit you. 4. Please indicate how important you feel these are. Veg Somewhat A Little lmpo nt important Important Important Unlm rtant (%) (%) (%) (%) °o) A. How important is it to celebrate holidays in the Puerto Rican way? 1.6 3.7 8.4 22.5 64.0 B. How important is it to raise your children with Puerto Rican values? 1.6 2.4 6.5 20.2 69.4 C. How important is itto maintain the use of the Spanish language? 1.3 2.3 6.0 15.1 75.3 D. How important is it to maintainPuerto Rican values as a part of your hfe? 1.3 1.3 5.5 15.9 76.0 Page 3. Some people have reported that other people do not treat them fairly in Southbridge simply because they are Puerto Rican. Others have not experienced this. How do you feel about the following statements? Please indicate the extent to which you strongly agree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree with the following statements. Neither ee Strongly Agr" Strongly Agree Agree Disagree Disagree Disagree (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) . 1n Southbridge, i feel that Puerto Ricans are not very welcomed. 12.6 22.5 34.8 22.8 7.3 . ln Southbridge, I feel that police often hassle Puerto Ricans. 22.3 24.9 29.6 18.3 5.0 In Southbridge, the presence of other groups that are not Puerto Rican makes me feel uncomfortable. 4.5 8.9 20.2 41.2 25.2 . In Southbridge, I feel Americans excluded me from activities because 1 am Puerto Rican. 6.8 13.9 28.7 34.2 16.3 In Southbridge parks, i feel Americans look down on me because I participate in customs ofmy culture. 11.1 14.7 27.1 35.0 12.1 Please indicate the extent to which you strongly agree, agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree with the following statements regarding your views on your cultural identity. Neither Strongly Nrr Strongly Agree Agree Disagree Disagree Disagree (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) . 1 strongly identify myself as a Puerto Rican first, rather than as an American. 66.4 18.5 10.9 3.9 0.3 Puerto Ricans in America should try harder to be American rather than practicing activities that connect them to their cultural heritage. 24.3 38.4 17.8 13.1 6.5 1 would prefer to be with Puerto Ricans, rather than with Americans. 13.0 9.6 45.3 5.0 1.3 . Puerto Rican culture is very distinct and very different from the American culture. 38.6 40.3 14.7 5.0 1.3 1 identify with other Puerto Ricans, even if they are not close friends or relatives. 31.8 49.3 12.9 3.9 2.1 1 would prefer to display the Puerto Rican flag, rather than the American flag. 22.3 18.6 39.6 14.4 5.0 155 Page 4. SECTION THREE: DEMOGRAPHICS _ 7. These final questions are about you. Please answer to the best of your knowledge. A1. How long have you lived in United States? Years (M = 19.53, gal = 10.91, min/max. = 0 yr. / 50 yrs) A2. Have you ever lived in Puerto Rico? No 21.2% Yes 79.8% Years (M = 18.54, ed = 11.14, min/max. = lyr. / 59 yrs.) CI Puerto Rico D United States D Other Bl. Where were you born? 72.9 % 27.1 % 0.0 % 82. Where was your father born? 99.7 0.3 0.0 B3. Where was your mother born? 99.2 0.3 0.5 B4. Where was your grandfather born? 98.4 0.8 0.8 BS. Where was your grandmother born? 98.7 0.5 0.8 C. What do you consider your cultural background to be? 8.2 % 0 Puerto Rican 5.3 C] More Puerto Rican than American 19.5 CI Puerto Rican and American equally 11.6 DMoreAmericanthanPuertoRican 55.4 0 American D. What is your sex? 39.5 % Male 60.5 % Female B. What is your age? (M = 34.27, g] = 13.96, min/max. = 14 yrs. / 80 yrs.) F. What is your marital status? 41.0 % Ci Single 45.2 % D Married 5.3 % Cl Separated 5.3 Ci Divorced 1.9 D Widowed 1.3 CI Other G. What is your work status? 64.6 % Cl Employed 2.9 % D Self-employed 10.9 % D home maker 5.1 CI Retired 6.9 D Unemployed 9.6 D Other H. What is your occupation? I. What was your total family income 1998 (before taxes)? [Include parents and children’s income] 8.1 % 0 Under $5,000 18.4 % Cl $15,000 to $24,999 9.7 % Cl $50,000 to $75,000 6.8 CI $5,000 to $9,999 17.2 CI $25,000 to $34,999 2.9 CI $75,000 to $99,999 14.9 0 $10,000 to $14, 999 21.4 C1 $35,000 to $49,999 0.6 CI $100,000 and over J. What is the highest grade that you completed in school? 16.8 % Cl Eighth Grade or Below 16.3 % D Some College 1.9% CI Some Graduate School 26.9 0 Some High School 6.4 D Associate’s Degree D Master’s Degree 27.7 C] High School Graduate/GED 4.0 CI Bachelor’s Degree D Doctoral Degree 156 Page 5. Thank you very much for taking the time to answer this questionnaire and for sharing your opinions with me. You have been a great help! Please return this survey to me in the enclosed self-addressed envelope. 157 d6 . Pu efios The Departm k, Recreation, and Tozpe‘ .1 ources ‘ I I‘ 'ty , g, MI 48824-1222 158 INSTRUCCIONES El questionnario propone capturar las Opiniones de la comunidad puertorriquefia dc Southbridge, Massachusetts. Este estudio intents obtener su punto de vista acerca de como los puertorriquefios interaccionan en sitios dc recreacidn pr'rblica. La persona cuyo nombre aparece en cl sobre debe ser e1/la que responds a1 cuestionario. Es importante que usted contests las siguientes preguntas lo mhs honestamente posiblc para que el estudio pueda representar correctamente la comunidad puertorriquefia. Por favor de coger su tiempo contestando las preguntas. El cuestionario debe cogerte desde 10 a 12 minutos para completar. Cuando usted termine, dobla el cuestionario una vez y ponlo dentro del sobre amarillo provisto para usted. No necesita ponerle un sello, simplemente envielo por correo. . Si usted recibio dos cuestionarios (uno en ingles y el otro en espafiol), por favor devuelva uno solamente. [Graciasl Cualquiera pregunta 0 problems comunicate con: Edwin Gomez, Doctoral Candidate Department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources 131 Natural Resources Building Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824-1222 Phone: (517) 353-9145 E-mail: gomezedw@pilot.msu.edu 159 Pagina 1. PRIMERA SECCION: PREGUNTAS DE LOS PARQUES 1. Se ha sugerido varios beneficios de tener parques en Southbridge. A delante se presenta una lista de oraciones. Por favor indique si usted estd muy de acuerdo, de acuerdo, ni de acuerdo ni en desacuerdo, en desacuerdo, a may en desacuerdo. Ni De Acuerdo Muy De De NI En Des- En Muy En _ . Acuerdo Acuerdo acuerdo Desacuerdo Desacuerdo A. Un beneficro dc tener parques en Southbridge es que permiten disfrutar de la naturaleea. D D D C1 D B. Ul'l beneficio dc tener parques en Southbridge es que ofrecen actividades recreativas. D C1 C1 C1 Cl C. Un bencficio dc tener parques en Southbridge es que chosen an sitio donde se puede socializar o establecer contactos personales. Cl D C1 C1 C1 D. Un beneficio dc tener parques en Southbridge es que ofrecen un sitio donde hacer ejercisio. E] El D D D E. Un beneficio dc tener parques en Southbridge es que ofrecen un sitio donde se puede compartir con familiares y amigos. C] D D . D C] F. Un beneficio dc tener parques en Southbridge es que ofrecen un sitio con espacios abiertos. D C1 Cl C] D G. Un beneficio dc tener parques en Southbridge es que ofrecen un sitio donde pueden ir niflos/jovenes. D 0 C1 C1 C1 2. En 1998, gCon cudntafi'ecuencia usted utilize las siguientes parques en Southbridge? lndr'que cudntas veces usted va a los parques por cada estacidn del ado en cada parque. A. MORRIS STREET PARK (Cmi Nuncs) (A veces) (rm) mm mm) (Muy We) Nunca Una vcz 2-4 veces 5-13 veces 14-20 veces 21-29 veces 30+ veces Verano (Junio-Agosto) D D C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 Otofio ( tiernbre-Noviembrc) D C1 C1 Cl C] D D Inviemo 'embre-Febrero) C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 Prirnavera (Mano-Mayo) Cl Cl C1 D D C1 C1 B. WEST STREET PARK (Cal Nines) (A veces) (Frecumte) (But-us Frequente) (Muy Preemie) Nunca Una vcz 2-4 veces 5-13 veces 14-20 veces 21-29 veces 30+ veces Verano (Jimio—Agosto) C1 C1 Cl C] D D Cl Otofio (Septiembre-Noviernbre) C1 C1 C1 D E] El Inviemo (Deciembre-Febrero) C1 C1 Cl C1 C1 C1 C1 Primavera (Mam-Mayo) C1 C] D D D C1 C1 160 Pagina 2. C. HENRY STREET PARK (Cui Nurse) (A veces) (Freer-cote) (But-rte Frequurtc) )(Muy W) . Nunca Una vez 2-4 veces 5-13 veces 14-20 veces 21-29 veces 30+ veces Verano (Junro-Agosto) C1 0 Cl C1 Cl C1 C1 Otofio ( tiernbre-Noviernbre) D D D C1 C1 C1 C1 lnvierno 'ernbre-Febrero) D C1 D D C1 C1 C1 Primavera (Mano-Mayo) D D C1 C1 C1 Cl C] D. WESTVILLE DAM RECREATION AREA (c-i Nncs) (A veca) (Freer-eats) (one We) (Muy W) Nunca Una vez 2-4 veces 5-13 veces 14-20 veces 21-29 veces 30+ veces Verano (Junio-Agosto) D D C1 C1 Cl Cl Cl Otofio (Septiembre-Noviernbrc) D C1 C1 C1 Cl D C] lnvierno (Deciembre-Febrero) D D C1 Cl C1 C1 0 Prirnavera (Mano-Mayo) C] D D C1 C1 C1 C1 E. HARRY J. McMAHON MEMORIAL FIELD (Dresser St. W) M) (BI-flue M) (May mic) Nunca Una vez 24w veces 5-13 veces 14-20 veces 21-29 veces 30+ veces Verano (Junio-Agosto) D D D D 0 C1 C1 Otoiio ( ticrnbre-Noviembrc) Cl D D C1 Cl D D lnvierno ernbre-Febrero) C] D D C1 C1 C1 C1 Prirnavera (Marzo- Mayo) C] Cl C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 SEGUNDA SECCION: PREGUNATAS SOBRE LA CULTURA PUERTORRIQUENA 3. Por favor indique curil(es) idioma(s) usted prefiere usar. M‘s Mhs alol Ambos In es Solo e For us Solo Espaiiol Ingles lgual Espaiiol Ingles A. Idioms més utilizado en la case. 0 C1 D D D B. Idioms preferido por radio y/o television. C1 C1 C1 C1 Cl C. Idioms preferido en revistas y/o periodicos. D C1 C1 C1 C1 D. Idioms preferido en la musica. D D C1 C1 D E. LQué idioms hablan sus ami os mes cercanos? C1 C1 C] D C] F. LQue idioms se habla en las estas y reimiones? D D D Cl C] G. gQué idioms hablan las personas que usted visits o que lo visitan a usted? El Cl E] D D 4. Por favor indique la importancia de los siguientes. M h Bastan an lmpgs't:ncia Importancla ImAdfiirltlhhcia Importancia lm pagoda A. fights importancia tiene ara usted cl cele- los dias feriados a] lo puertorriquefio? C1 C1 C1 Cl C] 3' tfiiifi’Wé gfi‘étifirie‘ifiifi“ D u a D u C' ‘fihi‘mm cit-.1 iieTguEJTespaaor? Cl 0 C1 C1 D D imamztssrsmssm o a a o a 161 Pagina 3. Algunas personas han informado que otras personas los(as) han tratado injustamente en las sitios priblico de Southbridge simplemente porque son puertorriquerios. 0tras personas no han sufrido esta experiencia. Quisiera saber su experiencia al respecto. Por favor indique en la medida en que usted estd: Muy de acuerdo, de acuerdo, ni de acuerdo ni en desacuerdo, en desacuerdo, o muy en desacuerdo con la siguiente. Ni De Acuerdo Muy De De Ni En Des- En Muy En _ . Acuerdo Acuerdo acuerdo Desacuerdo Desacuerdo En Southbridge, los puertornquefios no son bienvenidos. C1 C1 Cl C] D En Southbridge Ia policia molesta a los puertorriquellos. E] Cl C1 D 0 En Southbridge, la presencia dc otros grupos que no son puertorriqueiios me haw sentir incomodo. C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 En Southbridge los americanos me cxcluyen de sus actividades porque soy puertorriqueflo. C1 C1 C1 C1 C1 En Southbridge los americanos me rniran por encima del hombro (con menosprecio) debido a que yo participo en costumbres puertorriqueflas. Cl C1 C1 Cl C] Algunas personas sienten que las puertorriquer'ios deben adoptar valores americanos, otras sienten que puertorriquerios deben mantener sus costumbres y tradiciones. Por favor indique lo que usted opina de las siguientes oraciones. Ni De Acuerdo Muy De De Ni En Des- En Muy En Acuerdo Acuerdo acuerdo Desacuerdo Desacuerdo Me identifico primero como puertorriquefio en vez dc americano. D 0 Cl 0 C] Los puertorriqueflos en los Estados Unidos deben deesforzarsemdsporadaptarscalacultura americana en vez dc practicar actividades que los identifiquen con su cultural. D D D C] D Yo prefiero asociarme con pucrtorriquefios, en vez dc americanos. C1 D D C1 C1 La cultura puertorriquefia se distingue y es difercnte a la culture americana. D C1 C1 C1 C1 Yo me identifico con otros puertorriquefios aunque no seen amigos o familiares. C1 Cl 0 Cl Cl Yo prefer-la mostrar la bandera de Puerto Rico en vez de la bandera de los Estados Unidos. Cl . Cl C1 Cl [:1 162 Pagina 4. TERCERA SECCION: DEMOGRAFICAS 7. Estas rillimas preguntas son sobre usted. Por favor conteste segrin su mejor conocimiento. A1. gPor cuantos afios ha vivido usted en los Estados Unidos? Afios A2. gHas vivido en Puerto Rico? D No Cl Si -0 Afios Bl. gDonde nacio usted? Cl Puerto Rico 0 los Estados Unidos CI Otro BZ. gDonde nacio su padre? Ci Puerto Rico CI los Estados Unidos Ci Otro B3. gDonde nacio su madre? Ci Puerto Rico Cl 108 Estados Unidos D Otro B4. LDondc nacio su abuelo? C1 Puerto Rico CI 108 Estados Unidos D Otro BS. gDondc nacio su abucla? Cl Puerto Rico CI los Estados Unidos C1 Otro C. (A cuai culture usted pertenece? D puertorriquefio/a C1 més puertorriquefio/a que americano/a Ci arnbos por igual Cl mes arnericano/a que puertorriquefio/a Cl americano/a D. (,Cual es su sexo? Ci Hombre Ci Mujer B. Low es en edad? Aflos F. LCual es su cstado civil ? Cl Soltero/a Ci Casado/a CI Separado/a Cl Divorciado/a D Viudo/a D Otro G. 5Cua1 es su cstado ocupacional? . C1 Empleado/a Ci Empleado/a de 81 rrnsmo C1 Ama de casa Cl Retirado/a D Desempleado/a El Otro H. (,Cual es su ocupacibn? 1. gCuénto se gano su farnilia en 1998 (antes dc deducir los impuestos)? [Incluye las gananzas de padres e hijos.] CI Menos de $5,000 0 $15,000 to $24,999 0 $50,000 to $75,000 CI $5,000 to $9,999 CI $25,000 to $34,999 Ci $75,000 to $99,999 D$10,000to$14,999 D$35,000to$49,999 Cl$100,000y tabs 1. LCuél es el grado mas alto que usted ha completado? Cl Octavo grado o menos Cl Algunos creditos a nivel universitarios Cl Algunos créditos a nivel graduado CI Algunos creditos dc Escuela Superior 0 Grado Associado Cl Maestrfa D Diploma dc Escuela Superior CI Bachillerato Cl Doctorado 163 Pagina 5. Gracias por haber tornado de su tiempo para completar este cuestionario y por compartir sus opiniones conmigo. [Usted ha sido de gran ayuda! Por favor devuelva e1 cuestionario en el sobre predirigido. 164 APPENDIX B Map of Parks in Southbridge and Samples of Correspondence 165 / MAP OF SOUTHBRIDGE, MA AND ITS PUBLIC PARKS er "' r'rr r {:2 Downtown So’uthbrrdge / WavifleDanReaeatianAm(A)w ”H. ' Warflredei) /' 166 SAMPLE LETTER FOR COMMUNITY LEADERS December 8, 1998 Father Peter Joyce St. Mary’s Catholic Church 263 Hamilton Street Southbridge, MA 01550 Hello! I am writing you concerning a research project i am conducting involving the Puerto Rican community in Southbridge and their use of Southbridge parks. The study looks at possible things which might influence why Puerto Ricans (or other ethnic group members) in the United States may or may not use public recreation spaces. This research is required for the completion of my doctorate in the Department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources and Urban Studies at Michigan State University. I am asking for your help in soliciting names for my study. The phone book was going to be used as a list for mailing, but I realized that many Puerto Ricans are unlisted. I am approaching different community leaders to obtain names of people who might be interested in participating in the study. The participants will be required to give their name, mailing address, and whether they want the survey in Spanish or English. I would like to obtain names of both men and women. Please use the enclosed forms to collect the names. Their names are for mailing purposes only and will be taken off the list once their questionnaire is returned. This information WILL NOT be given to anyone. It will be kept extremely confidential. Participation is voluntary. I am asking that you please announce this to the Puerto Rican congregation at St. Mary’s. I will be in touch to follow up and offer any clarification that is needed. If you have any questions prior to my follow up, please don’t hesitate to call me at the number printed in the left margin. Thank you for any help you may have given me. Sincerely, Edwin GOmez Doctoral Candidate Michigan State University 167 SAMPLE OF SIGN-UP SHEET FOR SURVEY PARTICIPATION Por favor de escn’bir su nombre y direceidn si quieres participar en la investigacidn de puertom'queflos y el uso de las parques en Southbridge. Please write your name and address if you would like to participate in the study of Puerto Ricans and their use of parks in Southbridge. Gracias. Thank you. (1) Nombre: Direccién: o rid MA 1 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (2) Nombre: Direccién: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (3) Nombre: Direccibn: Mam Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (4) Nombre: Direccibn: S hbrid MA 1 50 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (5) Nombre: Direccibn: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (6) Nombre: Direccién: Spighprigge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (7) Nombre: Direccibn: Segthbrigge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: 168 (8) Nombre: Direccibn: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (9) Nombre: Direccion: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (10) Nombre: Direccibn: o hbrid Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: MA 01550 (1 1) Nombre: Direccibn: Somhbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (12) Nombre: Direceion: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (13) Nombre: Direccion: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: (14) Nombre: Direccion: Southbridge, MA 01550 Lenguaje preferido para el questionario: SAMPLE OF SPEECH FOR SPANISH-SPEAKING PARISHIONERS AT ST. MARY’S CHURCH DURING CHRISTMAS EVE MASS Bueno, quiero darle las gracias a Padre Peter por dejanne hablar aqui hoy. Y primero que nada, quiero desearle a todos una Feliz Navidad. Soy hijo de Chiqui y Julie Gomez. Estoy haciendo el doctorado en la Universidad Estatal de Michigan. Yo estoy conduciendo un estudio de los puertorriquenos en cuanto al uso de Ios parques de Southbridge y la influencia de la cultura puertorriquena. Este estudio es un requisito para la tenninacibn de mi doctorado en Parque, Recreacibn y Turismo. Yo sé que aqui hay otros Iatinos que no son puertorriquefios, y la razbn que estoy haciendo el estudio de puertorriquefios solamente es porque soy puertorriquei'io y me exigieron que no haga el estudio con otros grupos Iatinos porque yo no conozco la cultura de ellos. Bueno, (,porque estoy aqui hoy? Pues, porque necesito Ia ayuda y cooperacibn de todos Ios puertorriquel'los aqui presente. Yo necesito sus nombres porque muchos de ustedes no estan en la guia telefonica 0 en el censo. Voy a colocanne atras, cerca de la puerta de la entrada principal. Por favor paren, escriban su nombre y direccién en Ios papeles que estan en la mesa, o hablen conmigo si tienen algunas preguntas. Solamente puede dar el nombre si tienes mas de 17 afios. También, les quiero decir que el cuestionario lo van a recibir a fines de enero 0 al principio de febrero. Ya varias personas me han dado su informacibn, si algunos de udstedes estan aqui presente. pues no me tienen que dar Ia informacibn otra vez. Finalmente, les quiero decir que el cuestionario les va a coger de 10 a 12 minutos. Yo les voy a enviar el sobre con mi direccién y franque - haci que no tienen que pagar por mandarlo. Gracias por su tiempo y cooperacibn y [Feliz Navidadl 169 SAMPLE LETI' ER FOR ADMINISTRATION AT THE HIGH SCHOOL February 9, 1999 Mr. Walter Gosk Southbridge High School 25 Cole Avenue Southbridge, MA 01550 Dear Mr. Gosk: Greetingsl I would like to take this opportunity to thank you once again for agreeing to help me distribute the questionnaire to the Puerto Rican students at Southbridge High (SHS). In our previous conversation you had suggested that I include a letter stating what the study is about. This letter is enclosed in the box with the surveys. In addition to the letter of introduction, 1 have included a brief autobiography of specific events after I left SHS. l have provided you with 130 English surveys and 20 Spanish surveys. This was the number of surveys recommended by Mr. Bailey. The letters of introduction are also in English and Spanish. Please give the Spanish versions to the bilingual teacher. I understand that you may run into some difficulty in expediting the survey because of winter break. If this is the case, conduct the surveys alter break and get them back to me at the earliest convenient time. I have already spoken with Graciela, and she knows she will be receiving the box of surveys from you or your wife. If there are any questions, please don’t hesitate to call me at 517-353-5190. If you have access to e-mail, my address is gomezedw@pilot.msg.edu. Thanks again! Sincerely, Edwin Gemez Project Coordinator Michigan State University 170 SAMPLE LETTER FOR ADMINISTRATION AT HEAD START PROGRAM February 9, 1999 Mrs. Evelyn Velez Head Start Program 115 Marcy Street Southbridge, MA 01550 Dear Evelyn: Greetings! I would like to take this opportunity to thank you once again for agreeing to help me distribute the questionnaire to the Puerto Rican parents in the Head Start Program. In our previous conversation you had suggested that I include a letter stating what the study is about. This letter is enclosed in the envelope with the surveys. In addition to the letter of introduction, l have included a brief autobiography of specific events after I left Southbridge High School. I have provided you with the surveys and the list of people you said you would administer the questionnaire to. The letters of introduction are also in English and Spanish. The surveys are being sent out next week. I hope that you will be able administer all the surveys before the end of February. If you cannot, then have them back for me as early as possible in March. Please contact my mother (Julie) so that she can pay to have the surveys sent to me. This way it does not cost your program any additional cost. I have already spoken with my mother, and she knows she will be receiving the surveys from you. Her number is XXX- XXXX.. If there are any questions, please don’t hesitate to call me at 517-353-5190. if you have access to e-mail, my address is gomezedw@pilot.msu.edg. Thanks again! Sincerely, Edwin Gbmez Project Coordinator Michigan State University Enc (2) 171 SAMPLE PRE-SURVEY LETTER: ENGLISH (OTRO LADO PARA ESPANOL) February 5, 1999 «First» «Last» «Nums «Streets Southbridge, MA 01550 Dear «First»: Hello! I am writing you in reference to a study I am conducting that involves Southbridge Puerto Ricans and their use of Southbridge public parks. The study takes into consideration different things which may influence the use of public areas by Puerto Ricans in the United States. This research is the final requirement for my doctorate in the department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources and Urban Studies at Michigan State University. I am asking for your help. I have contacted different leaders in the Puerto Rican community in order to obtain names of people who might be interested in participating in my study. You (or someone who knows you) gave me your name so that you may participate in my study. Information provided by participants WILL NOT BE GIVEN to anyone. The information will be kept extremely confidential. Your participation is completely voluntary, and it will be greatly appreciated. Within a few days, you will be receiving the questionnaire. Until then, I have provided an autobiography. This introductory letter offers a brief history of my time away from Southbridge, far away from my Puerto Rican community. I hope that you find it interesting. Many thanks in advance! Sincerely, Edwin Gemez Doctoral Candidate Michigan State University 172 SAMPLE PRE-SURVEY LETTER: SPANISH (OTHER SIDE FOR ENGLISH) 5 de febrero del 1999 «First» «Last» «Num» «Street» Southbridge, MA 01550 Querido «First»: jHolaI Yo te estoy escribiendo en referencia a una investigacibn que estoy conduciendo a cerca de los puertorriquefios en Southbridge y el uso de los parques pr'rblicos de Southbridge. El estudio considera cuales son las cosas que infl en la utilizacién de sitios publicos por los puertorriquel‘los en Ios estados unidos. sta investigacibn es un requisito para la terminacibn de mi doctorado en el departamento de Recursos de Parque, Recreacién y Turismo y Estudios Urbanos en la Universidad Estatal de Michigan (Michigan State University). Yo le estoy pidiendo su ayuda. He contactado a diferentes llderes de la comunidad puertorriquefia para obtener nombres de personas que estarian. interesado en participar en mi estudio. Usted (o alguien que Ie conoce) me did su nombre para que participe en el estudio. La informacibn de los participantes NO SE VA A DAR a nadie. lnfonnacibn se va a mantener sumamente confidencial. Su participacibn es completamente voluntaria, y sera bien apreciada. Dentro de unos pocos dias, le va a llegar el cuestionario. Hasta que llegue el cuestionario, Ie he provisto unos datos autobiograficos. Esta carta de presentacibn ofrece una breve historia de mi tiempo fuera de Southbridge, lejos de mi comunidad puertorriquei'la. Espero que la eneuentre interesante. Hasta luego y imuchas gracias de antemanol Sinceramente, Edwin Gemez Candidato Doctoral Michigan State University 173 SAMPLE PRE-SURVEY AUTOBIOGRAPHY: ENGLISH (OTRO LADO PARA ESPANOL) Edwin Gbmez: From Southbridge, Massachusetts to East Lansing, Michigan l was bom in Southbridge on March 24, 1968: the third son of Natividad (Chiqui) and Julia (Julie) Gemez. Some of you may know my brothers Gerardo (Gery) and Julio (Cano) and my sister Graciela (Chela). My parents are both from Puerto Rico. Therefore, Spanish was always spoken at home. I have very fond memories of Southbridge. Perhaps my best memory is growing up in a community of Puerto Ricans. Regardless of where you went there was always a "bendicibn' given, an 'hola compai", or the pride one felt when hearing the word: Boricual Being the son of parents who emigrated from Puerto Rico I was raised in the old style. What my parents most wanted for us was a good education. I am happy to say that Southbridge’s school system prepared me very well for my college years. But, I get ahead of myself... After graduating fiom Southbridge High, Iwent to Bany University in Miami, Florida. This was my first time away from home, and I admit that I was a little scared. But, I did well at the university. After my scholarships ran out, I did not have the money to continue my studies. I went back to Southbridge and worked at the American Optical for a while. I worked in the same area of the factory where my mother used to work. I finally got to see and appreciate what my parents went through to provide for our family. I had a great time there. I met people from Gmece, Poland, and Puerto Rico. The job was respectable, but I still wanted to go to college. I decided to join the Anny. l was in the active Army for three years! i started as a private and left as a sergeant. The hours were long, but it was worth it The best thing about the Army was that I met people from all over the United States, and I Ieamed about different cultures. I had a lot of prestige and responsibility at the age of 22. The Army teaches you to stay focused on a goal. My goal was to go to a university and get a college degree. In the Army I earned the College Fund and GI Bill so I could go to college. This money allowed me to get my baccalaureate degree in Geographyfl’ourism at Salem State College. I went from there to Rochester, New York for my master’s in Tourism and Hospitality Management at the Rochester Institute of Technology. I am currently at Michigan State University working on my doctorate in Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources and Urban Studies. The scariest part of all my colleges was that I didn’t know what to expect. However, it was also the best part! I never thought I would get this far, but with love, faith and perseverance, I will make itl After this study is completed, I will finish my doctorate! My plans are to teach at a university. I was very fortunate that I had excellent professors to mentor me and encourage me. I would like to continue that tradition (perhaps with your children). I owe great thanks to God, my family and friends, my Puerto Rican community, and Southbridge schoolteachers. You have all helped me in many ways. Thank you! 174 SAMPLE PRE-SURVEY AUTOBIOGRAPHY: SPANISH (OTHER SIDE FOR ENGLISH) Edwin Gomez: Desde Southbridge, Massachusetts a East Lansing, Michigan Nacl en Southbridge el 24 de Marzo del 1968: B! tercer hijo de Chiqui y Julie Gomez. Algunos de ustedes conocen a mis herrnanos Gery, Cano, y Chela. Mis padres son de Puerto Rico. Por eso, en casa siempre se hablaba espaflol. Estoy aficionado de mis memorias de Southbridge. Ouizas mi mejor memoria es criandome en una comunidad de puertorriquel‘ios. Sin preocuparse de donde estaba, siempre habia una "bendicion", un 'hola compal”, 0 el orgullo que uno sentia cuando ola Ia palabra: jBon’cual Siendo hijo de padres que emigraron de Puerto Rico, me criaron al estilo viejo. Lo que mas querian mis padres para nosotros era una buena educacion. Siento alegrla en decir que las escuelas de Southbridge me prepararon muy bien para mis afios en la universidad. Pero, me estoy adelantando... Despues de graduarme desde Southbridge High, me ful a Barry University en Miami, Florida. Esta era la primera vez fuera de casa, y admito que estaba un poquito asustado. Pero, hice muy bien en la universidad. Cuando se acabaron mis becas, no tenia suficiente dinero para continuar mis estudios. Regrese a Southbridge y trabajo en la American Optical por un tiempo. Resulta que estaba trabajando en BI mlsmo sitio donde trabajo mi madre. Por fin pude ver y apreciar por lo que pasaron mis padres para mantener nuestra familia. Alli lo pase muy bien. Conocl a gente de Grecia, Polonia, y Puerto Rico. El trabajo era respetable, pero yo todavia queria ir a la universidad. Decldi alistarme al ejército. [Estuve en ei ejorcito por tres allos! Empezé como privado y sail sargento. Las horas fueron Iargas, pero valla la pena. Lo mejor del ejército fue que conocl a mucha gente desde diferentes partes de Ios Estados Unidos, y aprendi de diferentes culturas. Tenia mucho prestigio y responsabilidad a Ios 22 altos. El ejército te ensel‘ia cOmo mantenerse en foco a una meta. Mi meta era Ir a la universidad y lograr una educaciOn unlversitaria. En el ejército obtuve eI “College Fund" y '6! Bill" para Ir a la universidad. Este dinero me ayudo obtener mi bachillerato en GeograflalTurIsmo en Salem State College. Desde ahl me ful a Rochester, Nueva York para mi maestria en Manejo de Turismo y Hospitalidad en el Rochester Institute of Technology. Corrientemente, estoy en Michigan State University terrninando mi doctorado en Recursos de Parque, Recreacion, y Turismo y Estudios Urbanos. Lo que mas me asusto en todas las universidades fue que no sabla que me esperaba. Pero, [era también la mejor partel Jamas crela que hubiera alcanzado a tanto, pero con amor, fe y perseverancia, jiO hare! Después de completar esta estudio, terminare con todos los requisitos de mi doctorado. Pienso dar closes en una universidad. Afortunadamente, tenia profesores excelentes que fueron ejemplares y me apollaron. Yo quisiera continuar esa tradiciOn (quizas con sus hijos). Le debo muchas gracias a Dios, mi familia y amigos, mi comunidad puertorriquel'ia, y los maestros de las escuelas de Southbridge. Todos me han ayudado en diferentes maneras. [Graciasl 175 SAMPLE INTRODUCTION LETTER FOR SURVEY: ENGLISH (OTRO LADO PARA ESPANOL) February 9, 1999 «First» «Last» «Num» «Street» Southbridge, MA 01550 Dear «First»: Greetings! I am a Ph.D. student in the Department of Park, Recreation and Tourism Resources at Michigan State University. You are being asked to participate in a study. The attached questionnaire concerning participation in Southbridge public parks is part of a research project for my Ph.D. at Michigan State University. This study is concemed specifically with determining how residents in Southbridge view their public parks. The results of this study will help to provide crucial data to academicians and professionals interested in providing public leisure and recreation services to a diverse population. The United States (and Southbridge) is changing in terms of its ethnic and racial composition. Because of this, we need to better understand how people from different cultural backgrounds interact; how they feel about the environment they live in; and how this affects the use of public recreation settings. You or your friends or family have indicated that you would be interested in participating in this study. Only by obtaining the opinions of residents, such as yourself, can I accurately determine the Puerto Rican community’s point of view on Southbridge's public recreation areas. I guarantee that all information will be held in the strictest of confidence. The number at the lower, right hand corner on the front of the questionnaire is an identification number for mailing purposes only. This number allows me to identify who has responded to the survey so that I do not send a second survey. Once you have returned the questionnaire, your information is kept, but your name is removed from the database. Your participation in this study is voluntary. The survey should take about 10-12 minutes of your time. It will be appreciated if you can complete the questionnaire before February 28, 1999, and return it in the enclosed, self-addressed, stamped envelope. My sincerest thanks for your participation! Edwin Gomez Project Coordinator Michigan State University 176 SAMPLE INTRODUCTION LETTER FOR SURVEY: SPANISH (OTHER SIDE FOR ENGLISH) 9 de febrero del 1999 «First» «Last» «Num» «Street» Southbridge, MA 01550 Estimadola «First»: Saludosl Soy estudiante de doctorado en el departamento de Parque, Recreacion y Turismo en la Universidad Estatal de Michigan. Le solicito que participe en éste estudio. El cuestionario adjunto acerca de la participacion en los parques publicos de Southbridge es parte de un trabajo de investigacion para mi doctorado en Michigan State University. Este estudio intenta detenninar como Ios residentes puertorriquefios de Southbridge perciben sus parques pr’rblicos. Los resultados de este estudio ayudaran a proveer informacion para académicos y profesionales interesados en proveer servicios de recreacion pt’rblica para una poblacion multicultural. Los Estados Unidos (y Southbridge) estan cambiando en tenninos de su composicion étnica y racial. Debido a ésto, necesitamos entender como la gente de diferentes culturas interaccionan; como se sienten del ambiente en el que viven; y como esto afecta el uso de sitios de recreacion publica. Usted o sus amigos y familiares han indicado que estaria interesado en participar en este estudio. Con el analisis de sus opiniones podré deterrninar el punto de vista de la comunidad puertorriquefla acerca de Ios sitios de recreacion publica en Southbridge. Garantizo que toda la informacion sera confidencial. El numero en la esquina derecha del cuestionario es un numero de identificacion usado solamente para esta correspondencia. Esto sirve para poder identificar quién me ha contestado el cuestionario, para no enviarle otro. Tan pronto usted conteste el cuestionario, su nombre sera eliminado del banco de datos. La participacion en el estudio es completamente voluntaria. Te tomara alrededor de 10 a 12 minutos para completar eI cuestionario. Apreciaré si usted completa eI cuestionario antes del 28 do febrero del 1999. Por favor devuelva el cuestionario en el sobre predirigido que se Ie provee. [Mis mas sinceras gracias por su participacion! Edwin Gomez Coordinador de Proyecto Michigan State University 177 SAMPLE INTRODUCTION LETTER FOR FOLLOW-UP SURVEY: ENGLISH (OTRO LAoo PARA ESPANOL) March 19, 1999 «First» «Last» «Num» «Street» Southbridge, MA 01550 «First»: Hello! About a month ago, a questionnaire was distributed seeking your opinions on Southbridge parks and other Information. Unfortunately, there have been problems with the post office. Therefore, i know that you have not received the original survey, or have received it late and you were not able to answer It on time. As of today, I have not yet received your questionnaire. It is important that I receive your questionnaire In order to assure the success of this study and public recreation efforts. I obtained your name from friends and relatives because they thought that you would be interested in participating in this study. In order for the results of this study to be truly representative of the opinions of Puerto Ricans in Southbridge, it is essential that each person chosen for the study return their questionnaire. l have conducted this study because of the belief that Puerto Ricans in the Southbridge community could provide a very unique point of view on how different ethnic groups in the United States view their identity, community, and parks. The results of this study will help to provide information to professionals Interested in offering services related to public recreation for a diverse population. If you have not completed me questionnaire, I ask that you please take a few minutes to complete it. In the event that your questionnaire has been misplaced, a replacement is enclosed. Your participation in this study is completely voluntary and confidential. The survey should take about 10-12 minutes of your time. It will be appreciated if you can complete the questionnaire before April 4, 1999 (Easter Sunday). Please return the questionnaire in the self-addressed, business reply envelope (put it in your mailbox or drop it off at the post office). Postage is free! If you have already completed and returned the survey, please disregard this letter. Thank you for your cooperation in this very Important matter. Have a happy Easter and may God bless you! Sincerely, Edwin GOmez Project Coordinator Michigan State University 178 SAMPLE INTRODUCTION LETTER FOR FOLLOW-UP SURVEY: SPANISH (OTHER SIDE FOR ENGLISH) 19 de marzo del 1999 «First» «Last» «Num» «Street» Southbridge, MA 01550 «First»: [Holal Hace un mes atras, te envie un cuestionario pidiendo sus opiniones de los parques de Southbridge y otra informacion. Desafortunadamente, han habido problemas con el correo. Por eso, se que no he recibido el cuestionario, o lo recibio tarde y no ha podido contestarlo a tiempo. Hasta hoy, yo no he recibido su cuestionario. Es importante recibir su cuestionario para asegurar el exito de este estudio y los esfuerzos de la recreacion publica. Obtuve su nombre atraves de sus amigos y familiares porque ellos pensaron que usted estaria interesado en participar en este estudio. A fin de que Ios resultados de este estudio representen verdaderamente las opiniones de los Puertorriquenos en Southbridge, es esencial que cada persona escogida para el estudio devuelva su cuestionario. Hice este estudio con la cmenzia de que Ios Puertorriquenos en la comunidad de Southbridge pueden proveer un punto de vista unico acerca de distintos grupos étnicos en Ios Estados Unidos y como estos perciben sus identidades, comunidades, y parques. Los resultados de esta investigacion ayudaran a proveer informaciOn a profesionales interesados en ofrecer sus servicios en areas relacionadas a la recreacion publica para una poblacion diversa. En caso de que usted no haya llenado su cuestionario, Ie pido que por favor tome unos pocos minutos para completarlo. Si usted perdio el cuestionario, aqui le envio otro. La participacion en el estudio es completamente voluntaria y confidencial. Te tomara alrededor de 10 a 12 minutos para completar el cuestionario. Apreciaré si usted completa el cuestionario antes del 4 de abril del 1999 (“Domingo do Resurreccion”). Por favor devuelva el cuestionario en el sobre predirigido que se Ie provee (ponlo en su buzon o dejalo en el correo). No se necesita sellos, les gratis! Si completo y envio el cuestionario anteriorrnente, favor de no prestarle atencibn a esta carta. Gracias por su cooperacion en este asunto tan importante. Espero que tenga unas felices Pascuas y ique Dios le bendigal Sinceramente, Edwin GOmez Coordinador de Proyecto Michigan State University 179 APPENDIX C Conflrmatory Factor Analysis Concepts 180 CFA Concepts The purpose of this section is to provide the reader with an understanding of general confirmatory factor analysis concepts. It is meant to be as a conceptual overview, and as such, the mathematics will not be illustrated. The researchers primary concern is with measurement. The researcher has an idea of what constructs are to be measured, and this is the basis for the theoretical model. Theory indicates which constructs to measure. Before subjecting the data to analysis, a multiple factor measurement model was established in Chapter 3 (see Figure 10). Chapter 3 explained the indicators which were used for each of the theoretical constructs in the model on the basis of face validity. If the variables/Items do not meet face validity then it is unreasonable to expect the constructs to pass test of lntemal consistency and parallelism. The second step is to test the internal consistency for all constructs that have four or more indicators. The third step is to test for parallelism for all sets of indicators. In and example of three factors, T, U, and V, one can test the indicators of T with respect to U and V, one can test the indicators of U with respect to T and V, and one can test the indicators of V with respect to T and U. Discard indicators that do not comply with the internal consistency theorem, or the parallelism theorem, or both. Repeat steps two and three again, until a set of indicators that meet these three criteria have been developed. And when the set of indicators pass those tests, then and only then does it make sense to test the theoretical model. Implicit in these steps is an 181 understanding of the measurement model underlying the confirmatory factor analysis. The Measu ment M I In Figure 18, the underlying constmct is called Tme Score, usually abbreviated T. The word "True" in True Score is used in the sense that it is a faithful representation of a constmct. This is often referred to as a latent or unmeasured variable (Loehlin, 1987). The measurement model states that Tme Score drives various indicators (which are the X’s) and each one of those indicators is an imperfect indicator, that is, there is some error associated with it. This model assumes a linear scaling model. Figure 19. One Factor Measurement Model T (True Score)“ l/xl/Xl‘xx X1 X2 X3 Xk(lndicators) i i i e1 e2 e 3 ek (Errors) ' - True Score drives any number of indicators. If X1 were a perfect indicator, the correlation between True Score and X1 would be 1.0. The extent to which this correlation (between item and True 182 Score) is not 1.0, there is error. This is known as error of measurement. It is random response enor. Random response enor is some sort of non-systematic perturbation that affects the response. It keeps the correlation between item and True Score from being 1.0 (Frank J. Boster, personal communication, March 2, 1999). According to the classical theory of errors, if the (scaling) measurement model is correct, any observed score (any X) has two components: it is affected by True Score and it is affected by error (x = T + e ). For example, there are two reasons for a respondent to check ”agree“ on an acculturation item. One is his/her level of acculturation. The second is how much measurement error there is associated with that item. These two things determine where the check mark went on that item. The error is defined by whatever is not defined by True Score. The relationship between the indicator and True Score have a name. They are called factor loadings or parameters. The first step in a CFA is to estimate the parameters. Estimating Parameters in a Measurement Model: The Centroid Method This study uses Program CFA as the software program for the CFA portion of this study. Program CFA was initially developed by John E. Hunter at Michigan State University's Department of Psychology (April 20, 1985), and was revised by Hunter and Mark A. Hamilton of the Department of Communication at the University of Connecticut (May 11, 1992). Program CFA is a least squares, oblique multiple groups program that performs confirmatory factor analysis. It 183 uses the Centroid Method to estimate the parameters (factor loadings) for the measurement model. Factors are defined using communalities so that the factors are the actual constructs; i.e. defined without enor of measurement. Hunter’s Program CFA iterates three times using the Centroid Method. The factor solution stabilizes using communalities (item reliabilities) in the correlation matrix diagonal instead of ones in the diagonal. The use of communalities in the diagonal corrects for attenuated correlations due to measurement error by not assuming perfect reliability (i.e., an item correlating with itself perfectly) (Hunter & Gerbing, 1982). lntemal Consisteng Theorem The internal consistency theorem is employed to test the validity of the model. The internal consistency theorem states that the correlation between any two indicators of the same underlying True Score is the product of the correlations with True Score: It”: 17ro (3) The relationships that one observes in the model are all spurious because they are driven by their common cause: T. This model, a partial correlation model, predicts the correlation between the two variables that are related spuriously (X1 & X2) as the product of their correlations with their cause. So, the X1/X2 correlation should be, if the model is correct, equal to rm. rm. 184 Another way of saying this is that any two items (x3) should be correlated so that the correlation is equal to the product of the factor loadings. If the theorem fails, it suggests that "i" and "j" are not indicators of the same underlying factor, and to sum them would be a case of invalid measurement. Summation of the indicators can only occur if in fact this theorem held. Multi-factor Model Figure 19 illustrates a causal string with three constructs: T, U and V. There are three measurement models, one for each constmct. There is error in predicting U and error in predicting V. For T, the indicators are the X’s, and the errors associated with those indicators. For U, the indicators are Y’s. and the errors associated with those indicators. For V, the indicators are 23 and their errors. Figure 20. Multi-Factor Measurement Model 3“ iv T\ > u \~ > v AA?» [WEQ‘AY l/VN A ii’fifvfi Z'iii M 82 63 9; 31 The multi-factor model is an extension of the one factor model. In the multi-factor model case, there are more than one factor, and it is necessary to test the internal consistency theorem for each of the constructs (T , U, V). But 185 the multi-factor model alsomakes predictions about the indicators of the different factors. This prediction is derived from the parallelism theory. Parallelism Theorem The parallelism theorem states that the correlation between ultimate indicators of different factors (e.g., X’s are indicators of T, and Y's are indicators of U) would be the triple product of the correlation of X with its True Score T (its factor loading) and Y with its Tme Score U (its factor loading) times the correlation between the two true scores: r = rr xy xTyUTU (4) In a multi-factor model, a parallelism test is conducted for each pair of factors. That is to say, one can test the indicators of one factor and test if they are parallel with respect to indicators of another factor. If the items are parallel, this is further indication that the items measures their respective construct, and not any other construct. Another name for parallelism is external consistency. By testing parallelism, a more rigorous test of the content validity of ones measures is achieved. The test of parallelism is a more stringent test than internal consistency. It is important to impress upon the reader that a lot of the invalidity of an item does not show up until the parallelism test. If an item correlates higher with another factor than its own, then regardless of whether it is internally consistent, it is an invalid indicator because it correlates higher on another construct. 186 It is critical that the indicators are ultimate measures of the same factor/construct because if they are not, they should not be summed and correlated. If items pass both the internal consistency and parallelism tests, then it makes sense to sum the items. The factor/construct consisting of the summed items is correlated with the other constructs (factor correlation matrix). These are the correlations (corrected for attenuation) used in the path analysis. Confirrnatogy Versus Explorateu Facter Analysie CFA begins with a measurement model. It states that certain indicators, and only certain indicators, are indicators of a specific factor, and the other indicators are indicators of another specific factor. In an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) model, every indicator is driven by every factor (see Figure 20). Fundamentally, this is a major difference concerning how a factor is defined. Conceptually, in terms of partial correlations, in the CFA model, the correlation between X, and X2, controlling for T, should equal 0 (because it’s driven by a common cause). In a CFA, the Y,/Y2 correlation would also be zero when controlling for U, and so on. But when X and Y are correlated, the correlation will not be zero. A CFA is reflective of Tyson and Bailey's (1970) clustering approach to non-orthogonal solutions. In an EFA, the correlation between X’s and Y’s will be zero. With EFA, you correlate any X with any Y and control for all factors (F1, F2, F3...Fk). The partial correlation between the items, controlling for all the factors is zero. It is necessary to control for all factors in an EFA. If, for example, there were six indicators and two factors, the math would imply the model presented in Figure 187 21. Each factor is a linear combination of all the indicators, not just those that are said to be indicators of that factor. Figure 21. EPA Model F1 F2 / o’o’q I i” i I I I at e! 3 In CFA, a factor was defined as tme score. True score (an estimate) is defined as the sum of all its indicators that were valid measures of it. Theory guides the analysis. But, in an EFA, factors are summary variables. These summary variables are created in a non-theoretical fashion. There is no measurement model with which to start. These variables, in fact, are created to summarize all of the information in the correlation matrix. CFA starts with a model that makes substantive sense, and tests the notion that each set of indicators is driven by only one factor. Kline (1994) suggests that ”many psychologists believe that confirmatory factor analysis is in principle a superior method to exploratory factor analysis because it tests hypotheses, which is fundamental to the scientific method” (p. 80). 188 APPENDIX D Data Analysis Matrix and Chart 189 CORRELATION MATRIX OF 22 ITEMS USED IN FINAL CONFIRMATORY FACTOR ANALYSIS 3 b M ° BEN ACC DIS SID REC SES 3 4 5 e 3 11 12 14 19 21 22 23 24 25 29 27 so 31 32 34 35 3 .47 .51 .43 .15 .15 1e .13 —.01 .01 -.01 -.01 .05 .05 .07 .03 11 .17 .17 -13 -.13 4 .01 .53 .51 17 .13 1a .20 -.01 .01 -.01 ..01 .06 05 .09 .07 13 .19 .19 -15 -15 5 .01 .01 .55 .13 .20 .20 .22 -.01 .01 -.01 -.01 .06 09 .03 .03 14 .20 .20 —1e -13 e .01 .01 .01 .13 .13 .13 .2o -.01 -01 -01 -.01 .03 05 .03 .07 12 .13 .13 -14 -14 a .02 .01 .01 .03 .30 .30 .37 .25 21 .31 .29 .23 .25 37 .35 -.03 -.04 .-04 -15 -15 11 .03 .05 .02 .02 .02 .97 73 .27 23 .34 .32 .30 27 .40 .33 .03 -.05 —.05 -16 -13 12 .03 .02 .01 .05 .03 .01 .73 .27 .23 34 .32 .30 27 .40 .33 -.03 -05 -.05 .15 -16 14 01 .02 .05 .03 &&£_ .30 20 .33 .35 .34 .30 45 .42 -.04 -.03 -.06 -13 -1a 19 10 .04 .01 .03 .04 .04 .09 .07 41 .62 .57 .20 .13 .27 .25 ~.04 -.00 -.00 -.03 -03 21 .01 .04 .01 .01 .04 .03 .01 03 .03 .52 .43 .17 15 .22 .21 -.05 -.05 -.05 -.07 -.07 22 02 .05 .09 .03 .03 .03 .03 .oo .oo .02 .71 .25 23 .34 .32 -.05 -.07 -.07 .10 -10 23 04 .03 .04 .02 .02 .05 .01 .02 £$ 03 _ .23 21 .31 .29 -.05 -07 -.07 -.10 -.1o 24 .02 .02 .03 .03 13 .03 .02 .oo .02 04 .04 .03 27 .39 .37 01 .02 .02 -.11 ..11 25 .04 .01 .05 .01 .1o .05 .01 .03 .13 13 .13 1e .02 .35 .33 01 .02 .02 -10 -.1o 26 11 .00 .02 .07 .03 .04 .06 .05 .12 .03 12 .11 .01 .00 .49 02 .02 .02 —14 .14 27 .02 .03 .02 .03 .04 .00 .11 .03 04 .04 .08 io_1.o=2_ .02 .02 .02 —.13 -.13 30 .01 .05 .oo .02 .03 .01 .02 .oo .03 03 .02 .01 .05 I: 04 .02 _3—13—1 —.03 -.03 31 .07 .oo .07 .01 .02 ca .03 .01 .02 02 .03 .01 .01 02 .02 .03 .00 .43 -.05 -95 32 .09 .07 .03 .01 .01 .oo .01 .04 .04 .04 97 .02 .00 02 .07 .14 £é= .95 —.05 34 .05 .03 .oo .10 .02 .1o .05 .07 .04 .03 11 .04 .03 oo .03 .03 01 .09 .15 —: 35 .03 .02 .03 .06 .10 .02 .02 .07 .03 07 .02 .01 .14 02 .oo .03 .02 .09 .03 &= 3 4 5 0 a 11 12 14 19 21 22 23 24 25 29 27 30 31 32 34 35 BEN 35 .73 .79 .70 .25 .21 .23 14 -.09 -02 03 -.01 14 .05 .10 .03 .20 .31 .17 -.1e -.25 too 19 .22 .22 .27 .74 .31 .31 .90 .29 31 .45 .37 .43 39 .46 .40 -.02 —.09 —.05 -.14 -.27 01s -.00 -.01 .02 ..02 .33 .41 .36 .41 7o 59 .33 .31 .30 .44 .24 .29 -.09 .07 —.05 -.03 -.15 510 03 1o .13 17 .31 .53 .51 .57 .33 30 .43 .43 .54 .49 72 .93 -02 .07 .04 .13 -.23 age 24 .27 .29 23 -.07 -12 -.07 -.03 -.00 -14 -.06 -09 -.02 03 .04 .14 .52 .33 .31 —.04 .10 as —.23 —15 .25 -13 -32 -13 -19 -24 —.1o -.19 -.07 —.10 -23 .15 -13 .09 -.06 -.19 .07 .75 75 ' - Numbers in the first row and column (of upper matrix) represent the items. ° - Numbers above the diagonal (in upper matrix) are the PREDICTED correlations. ° - Numbers below the diagonal (in upper matrix) are the errors/residuals. ‘ - Numbers in the double line squares on the diagonal are inter-item correlations. and were used to assess internal consistency. ° - The lower matrix is the item by factor correlation matrix (used to assess parallelism), with bold numbers representing the factor loadings of each item on its factor. 190 LOCAL AND GLOBAL TESTS FOR THREE EPRP MODELS Individuel Link AnafgsLs Nen-Conetrainfi Link Em .z-v_alue mtg Original Model SID—>SES -.07 -.64 .522 ACC—*BEN -.20 m .027 ACC—>DIS -.10 -1.17 .242 DIS-+BEN -.10 -1.08 .280 ACC-+REC .16 1 .49 .136 Revised Model SID—>SES -.07 -.64 .522 SES-*DIS -.02 -.17 .863 SES—*REC -.06 -.55 .584 Final Revised Model SID—>SES -.07 -.64 .522 SES—vDIS -.02 -.17 .863 SID—>BEN -.02 -.19 .848 SES—>REC -.07 -.55 .581 DIS—>REC -.03 -.30 .764 Overall Chi Square for Each Model Model Chi Sggare g1 tvalue Original 10.09 5 .073 Revised .74 3 01 Final Revised .87 191 REFERENCES 192 REFERENCES Anderson, J.C., & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two step approach. Psychological Bulletin, ' fl (3), pp. 411-423. Allison, M. T. (1988). Breaking boundaries and barriers: Future directions in cross-cultural research. Mel of Leieure Reseerch, 10, pp. 247-259. Baldwin, J. A., & Bell, Y. R. (1985). The African self-consciousness scale: An africentric personality questionnaire. The Weetem Jeumel ef Black Studiee, 9 (2), pp. 61—68. Baron, R.B., & Kenny, DA. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Joumal ef Personalny ang Seciel Psyeholegy, 51 (6), pp. 1 1 73-1 1 82. Barona, A., & Miller, J. A. (1994). Short acculturation scale for Hispanic youth (SASH-Y): A preliminary report. Hiseenic Joumel of Behevieral Sciences, 1_e (2), pp. 155-162. Burch, W. R., Jr. (1969). The social circles of leisure: Competing explanations. Jegmel of Leisure Reseerch, 1, pp. 125-147. Burlew, A. K., & Smith, L. R. (1991). Measures of racial identity: An overview and a proposed framework. The Journal of Black Psycholegy, 17 (2), pp. 53-71. 193 Brown, RE. (1982). I he new New Englanders. Worcester, MA: Commonwealth Press. Carr, D. S., & Williams, D. R. (1993a). Understanding diverse recreationists: Beyond quantitative analysis. In P. H. Gobster (Ed.), Managing grben end high-ea fireation settings (pp. 101-106). Chicago, IL: USDA Forest Service North Central Forest Experiment Station. Carr, D. S., 8 Williams, D. R. (1993b). Understanding the role of ethnicity in outdoor recreation experiences. Joumel of Leieure Research 25 (1 ), pp. 22-38. Chavez, D. J. (1993). Vieiter Qrceetions of crewding and diecrimination at two nationel feaete in Segthern Celifomia (Research Paper PSW-RP-216). Albany, CA: USDA Forest Service Pacific Southwest Research Station. Christensen, J. E., & Dwyer, J. F. (1994, April 10-12). Examining African American and White outdoor recreation participation after demographic standardization on selected characteristics. In G. A. Vander Stoep (Ed.), Proceedings of the 1994 Northeestem Recreation Symmsium (pp. 159-161). Sarasota Springs, NY: USDA Forest Service, Northeastern Forest Experiment Station. Craig, W. (1972). Recreational activity patterns in a small Negro urban community: The role of the cultural base. Emnemic Geegraehy, 48, pp. 107- 115. 194 Cortes, D. E., Rogler, L.H., & Malgady, R. G. (1994). Biculturality among Puerto Rican adults in the United States. Amerign Journal of Communfiy Peycholegy, 22 (5), pp. 707-721. Cox, T., Jr. (1990). Problems with research by organizational scholars on issues of race and ethnicity. The rn lofA lied B havioral Science 26, pp. 5-23. Datz, B. (1998, December 13). Jobs fueled migration: Industrial Southbridge drew Puerto Ricans. Sunday Telegram, p. A23. Davis, J. A. (1985). The legic of gggl order. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. Dillman, D. A. (1978). ll and t I h n rv : Th Ides n methfi. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Duncan, 0.0. (1975). lntmdggien te etmctural eguation models. New York, NY: Academic Press. Dwyer, J. F. (1993, April 18-20). Customer diversity and the future demand for outdoor recreation. In G. A. Vander Stoep (Ed.), Proeeedings of the 1993 Northeastem RecLeetien Sympeeigm (pp. 59-63). Sarasota Springs, NY: USDA Forest Service, Northeastern Forest Experiment Station. Dwyer, J. F. (1994, August). Qgetemer givefi'fl and the future demand for outdmr reegetion (General Technical Report RM-252). Fort Collings, CO: USDA Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Forest and Range Experiment Station. Dwyer, J. F., & Gobster, P. H. (1992). Recreation opportunity and cultural diversity. Park& Recreation 27(9), pp. 22-33,128. 195 Echegaray, C. (1998, December 13). Commonwealth or State?: Island’s vote divides Puerto Ricans in Worcester County. Sunday Telegram, pp. A1, A22. Edwards-Hewitt, T., 81 Gray, J. J. (1995). Comparison of measures of socioeconomic status between ethnic groups. Psycholegical Remus, 77, pp. 699-702. Falk, J. H. (1995). Factors influencing African American leisure time utilization of museums. Mm_al of Leieure Reeearch. 27 (1 ), pp. 41- 60. Feagin, J. R. (1994). The continuing significance of race: Antiblack discrimination in public places. Amerign Sociolegigl Review, 56, pp. 101-116. Floyd, M. F. (1998). Getting beyond marginality and ethnicity: The challenge for race and ethnic studies in leisure research. Journal of Leisure Research 30(1), pp. 3-22. Floyd, M. F., Gramman, J. H., 81 Saenz, R. (1993). Ethnic factors and the use of public outdoor recreation areas: The case of Mexican Americans. Leisure Seienees, 15, pp. 83-98. Floyd, M. F., McGuire, F. A., Shinew, K. J., & Noe, F. P. (1994). Race, class, and leisure activity preferences: Marginality and ethnicity revisited. Journal of Leisure Research, 26 (2), pp. 158-173). Frazier, E. F. (1957). The black bogrgmisie. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Gordon, M. M. (1964). Assimilatien in American life. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 196 Gramman, J. H. (1996). Ethnic'mr, race. and outdoor recreation: A revi w of trends Ii 11 rch (Miscellaneous Paper R-96-1). Vicksburg, MS: Army Corps of Engineers, Waterways Experiment Station. Heise, DR. (1975). Wis. New York, NY: Wiley. Hendee, J. C. (1969). Rural-urban differences related in Outdoor recreation participation. The Jeurnal ef Leisure Reeearch, 1 (4), pp. 333-341. Henderson, K. A. (1998). Researching diverse populations. Joumel of Leisure Research 30(1), pp. 157-170. Hunter, J.E., & Gerbing, D.W. (1982). Unidimensional measurement, second order factor analysis, and causal models. Research i_n Organizational Behavior 4 pp. 267-320. Hutchinson, R. (1988). A critique of race, ethnicity, and social class in recent leisure-recreation research. Jeume! ef Leieure Research, 20 (1), pp. 10-30. Hutchison, R. (1987). Ethnicity and urban recreation: Whites, Blacks, and Hispanics in Chicago’s public parks. Journal of Leisrure Research. 19(3), pp. 205-222. lso-Ahola, S. E. (1980). The geie! geyeholegy ef leisure end recreation. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown. Jackson, R. G. (1973). A preliminary bicultural study of value orientations and leisure attitudes. Joumel of Leiegre Research, 5 (1 ), pp. 1 0-22. 197 James, L.R., 8. Brett, J.M. (1984). Mediators, moderators, and tests for mediation. Joumel ef Apelifi Peyehelegy, ee (2), pp. 307-321. Johnson, C. Y., Bowker, J. M., English, D. B. K., & Worthen, D. (1997). Theoreti l rs iv f thnic’ n t r r re ti n A review and eynthesis of Afrign-American partieipetion (General Technical Report SRS-11). Asheville, NC: USDA Forest Service, Southern Research Station. Johnson, C. Y., Bowker, J. M., English, D. B. K., & Worthen, D. (1998). Wildland recreation in the rural South: An examination of marginality and ethnicity theory. Jeumal of Leieure Research, 30(1), pp. 101-120. Kelly, J. R. (1980). Outdoor recreation participation: A comparative analysis. Leisure Scienge, 3 (2), pp. 129-154. Kenny, DA (1979). Qerrelatien eng eegselhy. New York, NY: Wiley. Kline, P. (1994). An eesy ggige te fa_cter enalysis. New York, NY: Routledge. Kline, RB. (1998). Prin i n ractice of tru le uation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford. Klobus—Edwards, P. (1981). Race, residence, and leisure style: Some policy implications. Leisgre SeienmeA (2), pp. 95-111. Lindsay, J. J., &Ogle, R. A. (1972). Socioeconomic patterns of outdoor recreation use near urban areas. Jouma‘l of Leisure Research, 4 (2), pp. 19-24. Loehlin,J.C. (1987). Letent veh'eble mflels. Hillsdale, CA: Erlbaum. Massey, D.S., & Denton, N. A. (1993). Amerign agertheid; Segregation and the making of the underclass. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 198 McKee, J. B. (1993). Seciolegy and the race problem: The fajure of e perspective. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. McMillan, D. W., 8 Chavis, D. M. (1986). Sense of community: A definition and theory. Jeumel of Cemmpnhy Psycholegy, 14, pp. 6-23. McMillan, J. B. (1983). The social organization of leisure among Mexican-Americans. Jppmal of Leiegm Reeeamh, 15 (2), pp. 164-173. Meeker, J. W., Woods, W. K., 8 Lucas, W. (1973, Fall). Red, white, and black in the national parks. North Amerign Revifl, pp. 3-7. Murdock, S. H., Backman, K., Colberg, E., Hoque, MD. N., 8 Hamm, R. R. (1990). Modeling demographic change and characteristics in the analysis of future demand for leisure services. Leieug Servige, 12, pp. 79-120. Murdock, S. H., Backman, K., Hoque, MD. N., 8 Ellis, D. (1991). The implications of change in population size and composition of future participation in outdoor recreation activities. Joprnel of Leieure Reeeerch, 23 (3), pp. 238- 259. Myrdal, G. (1944). An American dilemma. New York, NY: Harper and Brothers. Neulinger, J. (1981). The peyehelggy pf leisure (2"‘1 ed.). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, Publisher. Phillip, S. F. (1994). Race and tourism choice: A legacy of discrimination? Annale ef Touriem Reeearch, 21 (3), pp. 479-488. Phillip, S. F. (1997). Race, gender, and leisure benefits. Leisure Science, 19, pp. 191-207. 199 Puerto Ricans say ‘no’ to statehood. (1998, December 14). Southbridge Evening Newe, p. 13. Rabinowitz, J. (1998, December 15). Southbridge residents respond to Puerto Rican vote. Seuthhngge Evening Newe, pp. 1, 16. Resnicow, K., 8 Ross-Gaddy, D. (1997). Development of a racial identity scale for low-income African Americans. Johmel of Black Studies, 28 (2), pp. 239-254. Rodriguez, C. E. (1996). Puerto Ricans: Between Black and White. In C. E. Rodriguez, 8 V. Sénchez Korrol (Eds.), Historigel perepeetry‘ ee on Pperto Rican eurvivel in the Unitfl Statee (pp. 25-35). Princeton, NJ: Markus Weiner Publishers. Salant, P., 8 Dillman, D. A. (1994). Hew _te mngpet yeer ewn eurvey. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. Saldafla, D. H. (1994). Acculturative stress: Minority status and distress. Hispenie Jeumel ef Behaviohel Seiencee, 16 (2), pp. 116-128. Sanchez, J. l., 8 Brock, P. (1996). Outcomes of perceived discrimination among Hispanic employees: ls diversity management a luxury or a necessity? Acagemy of Menagement Jeumel, 39 (3), pp. 704-719. Schumen, H., 8 Gruenberg, B. (1972). Dissatisfaction with city services: ls race an important factor? In Harlan, H. (Ed.), PflQIQ end mljtig in urban sociehr (pp. 369-392). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. 200 Shaull, S. L., 8 Gramman, J. H. (1998). The effect of cultural assimilation on the importance of family-related and nature-related recreation among Hispanic Americans. Jopmel of Leisure Research, 30 (1 ), pp. 47-63. Shavelson, R. J. (1992). Statistical reasoning for the behavioral sciences (3" ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn 8 Bacon (A Simon 8 Schuster Company). Shinew, K. J. (1995), Gender, race, and subjective social class and their association with leisure preferences. Leisum Scienge, 17, pp. 75—89. Singh, B. (1981). Race, ethnicity and class: Clarifying relationships and continuous muddling through. Jeumal of Ethnie Stpgiee, 9 (2), pp. 1-19. Stamps, S. M., 8 Stamps, M. B. (1985). Race, class and leisure activities of urban residents. Journal pf Leieuge Reeearch, 17 (1 ), pp. 40-56. Stodolska, M., 8 Jackson, E. L. (1998). Discrimination in leisure and work experienced by a white ethnic minority group. Jopmel ef Leisure Research 30 (1 ), pp. 23-46. Stokes, J. E., Murray, C. B., Peacock, M. J., 8 Kaiser, R. T. (1994). Assessing the reliability, factor structure, and validity of the African Self- Consciousness Scale in a general population of African Americans. Journal of Black Psyeholegy, 20(1), pp. 62-74. Sugme, T. J. (1996). The Origine ef the Men ch’sis: Race and ingualm in mstwer Det_reit. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 201 Tanaka, J.S. (1987). "How big is big enough?": Sample size and goodness of fit in stmctural equation models with latent variables. Child Development, 58, pp. 134-146. Taylor, D. E. (1992). Urban park use: Race, ancestry, and gender. In P. H. Gobster (Ed.), Mehaging urban end high-pee aereatien settings (pp. 82-86). Chicago, IL: USDA Forest Service North Central Forest Experiment Station. Toth, J. F., Jr., 8 Brown, R. B. (1997). Racial and gender meanings of why people participate in recreational fishing. Leieure Sciences, 19, pp. 129- 146. Tyson, RC, 8 Bailey, DE. (1970). Clueter Anelyeie. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Wagner, J., 8 Soberon-Ferrer, H. (1990). The effect of ethnicity on selected household expenditures. The Smial Scieng Journal, 27(2), pp. 181- 198. Washbume, R. F. (1978). Black under-participation in wildland recreation: Alternative explanations. Leisure Scienge, 1 (2), pp. 175-189. Watson, M. A. (1992). Researching minorities. Journal of the Market Research Society, 34(4), pp. 337-344. West, C. S. (1994). Afro-centricity: Moving outside of the comfort zone. Journal of Physieel Egugtien, Rareetien, and Dance, 65(5), pp. 28-30. West, P. C. (1989). Urban region parks and Black minorities: Subculture, marginality, and interracial relations in park use in the Detroit metropolitan area. Leisure Sciences 11, pp. 11-28. 202 Wilson, W. J. (1996). When work disappeegs: The worid of the new urban poor. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf, Inc. Winter, P. L. 8 Chavez, D. J. (1998, May 29). Conducting research in the Hispanie commpnhy. [Handout]. Paper presented at The 7‘h lntemational Symposium on Society and Resource Management, Columbia, MO. Witt, P. A., 8 Ellis, 6.0. (1989). Th l i re ia nosti batte : Users Manual. State College, PA: Venture Publishing, Inc. Woodard, M. D. (1988). Class, regionality, and leisure among urban Black Americans: The post-civil rights era. Jopmal pf Leiepre Reeeerch, 20 (2), pp. 87-105. Xie, Y., 8 Goyette, K. (1997). The racial identification of biracial children with one Asian parent: Evidence from the 1990 Census. Seejel Forcee. 76 (2), pp. 547-570. Young, J.W. (1977). The function of theory in a dilemma of path analysis. Journal pf Applim Peychelegy, 62, , pp. 108-110. 203