man-A.» a. n- ~ « 3.».1.» :w.) a x 3 'u‘ “25.51. 4;: #5359543 .3 12 L1‘ 513' .o»....., . ”v.1 . .4. u.- m. ”Lid .33... . “izfii‘flgl‘ , : 2'} " ,. A ’v at 1 3' U ’ ‘ ’3‘ 7 2-3314: 3‘3f’.’s 9.. v D . .o—. w.“ I. ‘3.- it ‘ I“; «Rip «5563:; .i ’nfig. » 1.); ' .132 top”; . E "u! l ‘ a- v“ ; m. ..., ‘jfiz. Yaw. ....‘ g": .. ‘ mm. u m - 7 BJ‘ . PL'. ~ g 2 v . ' J" '1"! is : fl: 9 3 e . :9: z 33 " (gifgx‘vd' . ' I I ‘ . :— ‘ V N "woo.- “ 1 Annu- unor- ~ v- \f u. .vdv' wu- ~ \ , pa hurl-n ,» >4 A. ...N . «‘35 -q p- .135}?- 74‘3‘ " ..-2 iv? 79%.“.1'8 MICHIGAN 8 ATE U NERSWY UBRARIES m ”mull 1 gr Ill! Ill/UNI”!Hill/Ulllll/Wl ‘ 3 1293 1389 0235 This is to certify that the thesis entitled Dispute Management Training and Adolescents' Social Problem-Salving Competence, Social Self-Efficacy and Outcome Expectations presented by Stacy Elizabeth Curtis has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M-A. degree in P_svchologv Major professor Date April 10. 1996 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution “f“ . .0 .1 . PLACE ll RETURN BOX to remove this duckout from your record. To AVOID FINES mum on or baton date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU loAn AfflmativoMlONE Oppommty- Institution “ Wanna-o1 ‘ ~— - l r . DISPUTE MANAGEMENT TRAINING AND ADOLESCENTS' SOCIAL PROBLEM-SOLVING COMPETENCE, SOCIAL SELF-EFFICACY AND OUTCOME EXPECTATIONS By Stacy Elizabeth Curtis A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1996 ABSTRACT DISPUTE MANAGEMENT TRAINING AND ADOLESCENTS SOCIAL PROBLEM-SOLVING COMPETENCE, SOCIAL SELF-EFFICACY AND OUTCOME EXPECTATIONS By Stacy Elizabeth Curtis This study evaluated the effects of a violence prevention program on middle school students in a Midwestern metropolitan area. It was proposed that conflict resolution training would directly impact students’ perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations of nonviolent conflict resolution, which would affect students’ social problem-solving competence. Social problem-solving competence was proposed to impact nonviolent conflict resolution. Interviews were conducted with 38 students who received dispute management training, 65 students who attended an intervention school but did not receive training, and 108 students who attended a control school. A model was tested using hierarchical regression analyses. Results of this study do not appear to support the efficacy of this dispute management program. However, findings confirm previous research which suggests that both self-efficacy and outcome expectations are important indicators of pro-social behavior. These factors should be considered in future program development and evalutation. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank my advisor Dr. William Davidson and my committee members Dr. Deborah Salem and Dr. Timothy Bynum for their advice, time, and support. I need to thank Charlene Baker and Martha Ehle for their time, willingness, and intelligence in coding the many responses from which this thesis grew -- they were truely reliable. I also need to thank Becki Campbell for her assistance and statistical knowledge. Next, I wish to thank my friends who lived the moments of this thesis (and graduate school) with me. Alison, Cheryl, David, Susan and many other critical supporters, without your understanding, empathy, help and Shared laughs this work would never have been completed. I wish you all the best. Thanks to my mother and father, Pam and Ross Curtis, for seeing me through. I thank you for your unfailing love, support, and understanding. To my brother, Scott -- I wish you the world. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ........................................ viii CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION .......................................... 1 Violence Prevention and Conflict Resolution .......................... 8 Theoretical Background ................................... ll Biogenetic ......................................... 12 Social Control ....................................... 13 Ecological ......................................... 15 Social Leaming Theory ................................. 16 Program Design - Conflict Resolution as Social Problem-Solving ...... 18 Social Problem-Solving Competence ...................... 23 Perceived Self-Efficacy .............................. 26 Expectations of the Outcomes of Competent Behavior ........... 28 Summary ....................................... 30 We ................... 31 Goals and Hypotheses ....................................... 35 CHAPTER TWO METHOD .............................................. 38 Setting ................................................. 38 Subjects ................................................ 40 Design ................................................. 43 Program Description ........................................ 44 Procedure ............................................... 47 Measures ............................................... 49 Development of Coding Manuals ............................. 51 Social Problem-Solving Competence ........................ 52 Outcome Expectations .................................. 56 Perceived Self-Effieacy ................................... 58 Nonviolent Conflict Resolution .............................. 59 iv CHAPTERTHREE RESULTS .............................................. 62 Analysis of Variance ........................................ 62 Hypothesis 1 .......................................... 62 Hypothesis 2 .......................................... 64 Hypothesis 3 .......................................... 64 Hierarchical Regression Model Predicting Social Problem-Solving Competence ................................. 65 Hypothesis 4 .......................................... 65 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis ..................... 74 Summary of Results ........................................ 74 CHAPTER FOUR DISCUSSION ............................................ 77 Summary ............................................ 82 Future Directions ....................................... 84 APPENDICES Appendix A: Coding Manual for Scenarios -- Social Problem-Solving Competence ............................... 86 Appendix B: Coding Manual for Scenarios -- Outcome Expectations ......... 95 REFERENCES .......................................... 101 LIST OF TABLES Table l - Students Receiving Training in Dispute Management Skills -- Self-report Data and Data of Homeroom Assignment ........................ 42 Table 2 - Content of Seven Interview Scenarios Presented to Students ......... 50 Table 3 - Reliability Analysis for Social Problem-Solving Competence ........ 55 Table 4 - Reliability Analysis for Expectations of the Outcome of Competent Behavior ..................................... 55 Table 5 - Reliability Analysis for Perceived Self-Efficacy ................ 55 Table 6 - Content of Questions Related to Conflict and Antisocial Behaviors - Interview ........................... 61 Table 7 - Comparative Analysis of Social Problem-Solving Competence, Social Self- Efficacy, Outcome Expectations and Nonviolent Conflict Resolution by Experimental Condition Using One-way Analysis of Variance ........... 63 Table 8 - Hierarchical Multiple Regression Anlysis and Intercorrelations Predicting Perceptions of Self-Efficacy - Equation 1 ........................ 68 Table 9 - Hierarchical Multiple Regression Anlysis and Intercorrelations Predicting Outcome Expectations - Equation 2 ........................... 68 Table 10 - Hierarchical Multiple Regression Anlysis and Intercorrelations Predicting Social Problem-Solving Competence - Equation 3 .................. 69 Table 11 - Hierarchical Multiple Regression Anlysis and Intercorrelations Predicting Nonviolent Conflict Resolution - Equation 4 ...................... 71 Table 12 - Hierarchical Multiple Regression Anlysis and Intercorrelations Predicting Nonviolent Conflict Resolution - Equation 4 Backwards .............. 73 vi Table 13 - Content of Questions Included in Survey Used During Wave One of the Overall Evaluation Project ................................. 79 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 - Proposed Model .................................... 37 Figure 2 - Revised Model ..................................... 76 viii Chapter One INTRODUCTION Violence in schools has increased in recent years. We hear nightly on the news about children carrying guns to schools, about drive-by shootings, and about critically injured teenagers. During a six month period of the 1988-1989 school year, over 400,000 students nationwide were victims of violent crimes at school including assault, rape, and robbery (National Center for School Safety, 1991). In a study conducted by the Centers for Disease Control (1992), 50% of boys and 25 % of girls reported having been physically attacked by someone at school. Furthermore, the National Crime Victimization Survey of 21,554,092 students reported that during a Six month period nearly one tenth of students, ages twelve to nineteen years old, were victims of crime in or around school. Of these students, two percent (431,081) were victims of violent crime (Bastian & Taylor, 1991). The above statistics indicate that our schools have become violent, unsafe places for children. Other research provides further evidence of such violence. Osofsky (1995) conducted a survey with African-American parents and children living in an inner-city environment with a high rate of police documented violence. Results indicated that 35 % of parents felt that their children were not safe walking to school and 54 % perceived that their children were not safe playing in their neighborhoods. 1 2 Only 17 96 of parents surveyed believed their children were very safe while participating in these activities. The majority of parents (62 %) felt their children were very safe at home, while 30% of parents felt that their children were very safe at school. It is alarming to recognize that 70% of parents surveyed perceived that their children were not very safe at school. Finally, 90% of parents in this inner-city sample indicated that violence was a critical problem or crisis in their neighborhood. According to official records of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (1990), homicide has become the second leading cause of death for 15 to 24 year-olds and the leading cause of death for black youth (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Hausman, Spivak, Prothrow-Stith, & Roeber, 1992). Homicide is currently the leading cause of death for young African-American men ages 15 to 34, with a rate ranging between five and ten-times higher than the rate for white men (DeJong, 1994). For African-American men between the ages of 15 and 19, the rate of homicides involving firearms more than doubled between 1984 and 1988 (DeJong, 1994). Rates of homicides in large cities are even more alarming. In Detroit, for example, the homicide rate for African-American males ages 15 to 19 quadrupled between 1987 and 1988 (DeJong, 1994). It is striking to note that while the FBI’S Uniform Crime Report for 1994 indicates a national decrease in all types of crime for the third year in a row, it reports an increase in crime among juveniles under age eighteen (Smith, Beals, Brant, & Annin, 1995). A report of the Department of Justice (as cited in Smith et a1. , 1995) showed that the number of adolescents arrested for serious offenses rose from 83,400 in 1983, to 129,600 in 1992. It is beyond dispute 3 that violence presents a major crisis in our communities, among our youth, and in our schools. Aggressive and violent acts such as verbal threats, harassment, theft, and physical assaults drastically disrupt the school and learning environment and can cause fear and intimidation in students (Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987). Anti-social and aggressive behavior interferes not only with school learning but also inhibits the development of positive peer relations in the school context (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993). The National Crime Victimization Survey indicated that twenty percent of all students surveyed feared being attacked at school (Bastian & Taylor, 1991). The CDC (1992) also surveyed students and found that during the month prior to being surveyed eight percent of students in the ninth through twelfth grades had been in at least one physical fight resulting in injuries that required medical attention. The CDC survey found that an estimated eighteen fights per 100 students occurred per month and that more male Students (28 %) than female students (7%) were involved in physical fights. Furthermore, the highest rates of physical fights were reported among African-American male students (47%) followed by Hispanic males (35 %) and Anglo-American males (22%). The Commission on Violence and Youth (1993) reported findings from the National Research Council Showing that four characteristics of schools may contribute to violence. These characteristics included: (1) relatively high numbers of students occupying a limited amount of space, (2) reduced ability to avoid confrontations, (3) imposed behavioral routines and conformity which may contribute to feelings of anger, 4 resentment, and rejection, and (4) poor building design features which may facilitate the commission of violent acts. Although violence is not limited to urban environments, and indeed in recent years has increased drastically in suburban schools, trends Show that violence is most prevalent in large, urban schools in areas of lower socioeconomic status that often experience overcrowding and a lack of resources (Elliott, 1994; Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987). Personal violence, furthermore, is most prevalent in inner-city junior-high schools with large minority populations (I-Iausman et al., 1992; Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987). According to comparison studies of school records, schools with higher incident rates of violence tend to have lower levels of Student academic achievement, higher rates of absenteeism, and more school drop-outs (Christie & Toomey, 1990; Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Hellman & Beaten, 1986). The comparatively high incidence of violence in inner-city schools seems to be related to social factors such as poverty or low socioeconomic conditions, discrimination, and oppression in the surrounding neighborhoods and does not seem to be related to race or ethnicity (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Elliott, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). In addition, the high incidence of violence in inner- city schools is also related to chronic exposure of school children to violence in the surrounding communities (Osofsky, 1995). Furthermore, the availability of handguns and the use of alcohol and other drugs are also considered to be Strong correlates of inner-city adolescent violence (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; DeJong, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). 5 Contrary to common perceptions and media portrayals that violence is random and that victims of violence are victims of unknown assailants, most violent acts, including homicides, occur between people who are acquainted or related to each other (DeJong, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). In a school setting, therefore, most violent and aggressive acts occur between students who know each other (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Hausman et al., 1992; Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987). In a larger context, the majority of interpersonal violence between adolescents involves same race and age peers and most frequently involves people who are acquaintances or friends (Hausman et al., 1992; Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987). As outlined above, the causes and characteristics of interpersonal violence in schools are multiple, complex, and often hard to separate from the larger social and cultural system (Elliott, 1994; Osofsky, 1995; Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). As a result, high rates of violence in schools cannot be attributed to a single factor, but instead are related to numerous environmental catalysts including poverty, access to guns, use of alcohol and other drugs, and gang activity (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; DeJong, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). These factors have contributed to a situation in which reducing school violence has become one of the most challenging tasks facing educators, legislators, parents, psychologists, and students (Hranitz & Eddowes, 1990). Studies have found that the responses and attitudes of a school' S administration can promote and reinforce students' attitudes towards violence (N uttal & Kalesnik, 1987). School administrators ean strive actively to remove violence from the school environment by promoting and reinforcing students' nonacceptance of violence. 6 Similarly, student perceptions of an administration as consistent and firm in its rule enforcement have been found to contribute to a reduction in rates of school violence (Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987). In the past decade, attempts to reduce violence in schools have increased in response to increasing violence. Schools have implemented a variety of programs (both structural and educational) to enhance a sense of safety and to curtail violence on their campuses (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b; Webster, 1993). Attempts to reduce violence in school environments have included physical changes such as installing metal detectors at entrances to schools, altering bathrooms and school hallways, introducing guards into schools, and using intercom systems to speed communication between classrooms and administration offices (DeJong, 1994; Nuttal & Kalesnik, 1987; Webster, 1993). Programmatic attempts to reduce violence have included implementing conflict resolution and violence prevention programs designed to teach and encourage students to use nonviolent methods of resolving interpersonal disputes (N uttal & Kalesnik, 1987; Prothrow-Stith, 1991). Such programs stress that conflict is a normal part of interpersonal relationships and teach students Skills to manage conflict constructively. Because Statistics indicate that violence occurs more often between adolescents who know each other, programs aimed at reducing adolescent violence may be less effective if they do not address the interpersonal relationship aspect of violence (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). In response to the growing concern among educators, in recent years, there has been a rapid rise in the implementation of violence prevention and conflict resolution _"_'-__r-P 7 curricula and programs in schools (Johnson & Johnson, 1995; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a; Webster, 1993). Currently, such programs exist in thousands of elementary, middle, and high schools nationwide. Furthermore, some states are considering making conflict resolution programs part of the mandatory curriculum in their public schools (Webster, 1993). The widespread adoption of programs reflects belief in their apparent successes (Johnson & Johnson, 1995 ; Webster, 1993). Furthermore, available findings of research and program evaluations indieate promising directions for intervention and prevention strategies (Roth, 1994). However, much of the support for intervention is based on anecdotal accounts of programs and very little empirical evidence exists as to whether conflict resolution or violence prevention programs are effective at reducing violence (Johnson & Johnson, 1995 ; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a; Webster, 1993). Therefore, further research is necessary to determine whether conflict resolution programs are an effective means of reducing violent responses to interpersonal conflict. '5.— “0"“ This thesis describes an evaluation of a dispute management program. Before the description of this study is presented it is important to review definitional issues related to this field of research. In the research literature, the terms conflict resolution, violence prevention, conflict intervention, and dispute management are used interchangeably to describe the same or similar intervention and prevention programs. In addition, the terms violence and aggression are also used interchangeably (Tate, Reppucci, & Mulvey, 1995). Finally, both aggression and violence can be found in the literature on delinquency, which can be defined as ”a heterogeneous concept including behaviors as diverse as theft, burglary, robbery, vandalism, violence against persons 8 [and] drug use” (Farrington, 1987, p. 33). Research on all of these areas was included in the literature review conducted for this investigation. The focus of this study is to understand how dispute management training can reduce violence in schools. The goals of this Study are (l) to evaluate the effects of a dispute management training program, and (2) to explore the relationships between dispute management training and the theoretical processes and factors thought to be salient in reducing violent conflict. These factors include students' prosocial competence in social conflict Situations, perceptions of self-efficacy for competent behavior, and expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior in interpersonal conflict Situations. In the following sections, an overview of violence prevention and conflict resolution programs is presented. This discussion is followed by a review of important theories related to violence and conflict, with a strong focus on Social Learning Theory (Bandura, 1977). The constructs of prosocial problem-solving competence, perceived self-efficacy, and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior are discussed, and research related to these constructs is presented. Finally, the hypotheses of the current study are presented. Violence Prevention and Conflict Resolution Multiple school studies have determined that a majority of interpersonal conflicts in schools results from gossip, disputes over property, invasion of privacy, verbal arguments, and dirty looks (Araki, 1990; Cameron & Dupuis, 1991; Prothrow-Stith, 1987). It is often these types of altercations which escalate into violent confrontations between students and may cause severe injuries or even death. Arguments are the 9 leading precipitant of homicide -- approximately 50% of all homicides follow arguments, compared to only two percent that involve gang activity or sexual assault (Prothrow-Stith & Spivak, 1992). The Violence Prevention Project (VPP), a community-based outreach and educational project in Boston, conducted a survey in 1987 with 815 students (57.6% African—American, 25.3 % Latino) from nine high schools across the United States. It found that 9.3 % of the students had been physically attacked and hurt at school during the previous month (DeJong, 1994). More than half of the students reported being in a physical fight with someone their own age during the past six months; approximately one in ten Students reported being in a physical fight during the previous week, although 38.7% said that they had been in a situation during the past week where they might have gotten into a fight. The interpersonal conflict referred to by students most often involved a friend (23.9%), a family member (14.1%), or someone else they knew (30.3%) (DeJong, 1994). Although nationally the actual percentages of students experiencing these types of violence at school may be relatively low, a few violent acts can have deleterious effects for a large number of students (Short, 1990; Wolfgang, Figlio, & Sellin, 1972). For example, one study found that students who bring weapons to school report doing so in order to protect themselves from being victimized (National Center for School Safety, 1991). The perceived need for protection and the availability of guns has led to an increase in the number of firearm related youth homicides from 1984 to 1989 10 (Fingerhut, Ingram, & Feldman, 1992). A national study conducted in 1990 by the CDC revealed that nearly 20% of all students in grades 9 through 12 reported carrying a weapon at least once during the past month. AS a result of the prevalence of guns and interpersonal violence, many schools struggle to provide a safe learning environment. Conflict resolution and violence prevention programs are a step toward increasing Student safety by trying to reduce school violence. Conflict resolution programs are based upon the tenets that (l) violence is a product of interpersonal conflict and (2) that violence is a learned response to conflict. A major goal of violence prevention and conflict resolution programs is to teach and foster students' use of nonviolent and constructive methods of resolving interpersonal disputes. In order to do so, programs present students with the skills required to manage conflicts constructively along with the opportunities to practice and master these skills. Programs differ in their specific approaches to teaching this information. Many programs present students with information on the risks of victimization while challenging students' attitudes that support the use of violence by teaching means-end relationships, consequences of social actions, and alternative strategies for resolving interpersonal problems (Crary, 1992; Prothrow-Stith, 1987; Webster, 1993). Although empirical evaluations of intervention efforts are lacking, schools that have implemented conflict resolution or dispute management programs report fewer incidences of escalation of interpersonal conflicts between students (Hranitz & Eddowes, 1990; Prothrow—Stith, Spivak, & Hausman, 1987; Webster, 1993). Schools with these programs also report positive changes in the overall school environment. 11 Among these changes are increased satisfaction with the school climate reported by students and teachers, increased feelings of safety on school property, increased levels of self-esteem among students, and student reports of feeling more able to resolve conflicts constructively and nonviolently (Hranitz & Eddowes, 1990; Prothrow-Stith et a1. 1987; Webster, 1993). Research has Shown that the instructional use of conflict can lead to increases in achievement, higher level reasoning, and creative problem solving (Webster, 1993). Despite these findings, Johnson, Johnson, Dudley, and Acikgoz (1994) noted that few evaluation studies of conflict management programs have been conducted without methodological weaknesses, and therefore results are tenuous. Given that the evaluation of conflict management programs is still developing it is relevant to consider multiple approaches towards examining the effects of such programs. Furthermore, when trying to understand and reduce violence on school campuses it is relevant to explore theories on the underlying causes of violent behavior. WW There are multiple theoretical approaches which attempt to explain the risk factors and causes of violence. The terms violence and aggression are used interchangeably in the literature (Tate et al. , 1995) and, therefore, theories of aggression are relevant to a discussion of violence. Furthermore, both aggression and violence are considered to be behaviors characterized as delinquent (Farrington, 1987) and, therefore, theories on the development of delinquency are also relevant. Many theories explain violence in terms of a multitude of individual and social factors operating in the development of violence, delinquency, and other related 12 problems (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). The numerous suggested psychosocial risk factors of violence include: poverty, exposure to violent or criminal activity, lack of positive role models, lack of commitment to social norms, pregnancy or birth complications, harsh or erratic early family experiences, hyperactivity or impulse control problems, inability to defer gratification, impaired cognitive functioning and low academic achievement, poor peer relations skills, and poor coping skills (Clark, 1994; Elliott, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). Theorists incorporate different psychosocial risk factors into their explanations of violence, aggression, and delinquency. The theories included here review the biogenetic, social control, ecological, and social learning views of violence. Biogenetic Several different biological and neurological processes are hypothesized to be linked to violent behavior (Elliott, 1994; Tate et al., 1995). Among these biological factors are genetic influences, neurological impairments, and the effects of neurotoxins (Elliott, 1994; Tate et al., 1995). It is difficult to separate the respective roles of inherited factors, acquired biological factors (e.g. , birth trauma, head injuries), and learned psychosocial factors in the development of violence (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Zigler, Taussig, & Black, 1992). Acquired biological deficits, such as the effects of low birth weight and other prenatal and perinatal complications, including exposure to neurotoxins such as lead, have all been linked to or associated with lower intelligence, decreased attention levels, increased physical activity, and the prevalence of violent behavior (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Elliott, 13 1994; Zigler et al. , 1992). Furthermore, antisocial personality traits, attention deficit or hyperactivity disorders, a fearless and impulsive temperament, and serious head injuries have also been Shown to be related to violent behaviors (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Elliott, 1994; Tate et al., 1995). Researchers of biological and pharmacological interventions argue that such interventions may improve the effectiveness of psychological approaches to treatment and, therefore, may be effective in helping to reduce violence (Tate et al. , 1995). However, current understandings of the biological relationships to violence are limited (Tate, et al. , 1995), and many questions remain as to the interaction of nature and nurture in the development of violence. Therefore, biological considerations may be most useful when considered as part of a multidisciplinary approach to violence prevention (Zigler et al., 1992). mm According to control theory, low social control leads to delinquency because it provides the adolescent with the freedom to deviate from socially desirable behaviors (Agnew, 1993). Social control theories assume that delinquency results when an individual's bonds to society are weak or broken (Hirschi, 1971). According to Hirschi (1971), there are four forms of social control or social bonding, including: ( 1) attachment, defined as the affection and respect an adolescent holds toward significant others, such as parents and teachers; (2) commitment, which refers to an adolescent's actual or anticipated investment in conventional activities; (3) involvement, the amount of time an adolescent spends engaged in conventional activities; and (4) beliefs, defined 14 as an adolescent' s commitment to the central value system of the society. According to social control theory there is a negative correlation between these four forms of social bonding and delinquency (Agnew, 1993). Research on control theory indicates that individuals low in attachment, commitment, involvement, and belief are more likely than other individuals to engage in delinquent behavior (Agnew, 1993). Most control theorists assume that all individuals possess unfulfilled needs (Agnew, 1993) and, therefore, have the potential to act in a delinquent manner. Most individuals, however, do not engage in delinquent behaviors. It is when control is low (i.e. , when an individual is low in attachment, commitment, involvement, and belief) that he or She is free to satisfy unfulfilled needs in the most readily available way, which may include engaging in delinquent activity (Agnew, 1993; Hirschi, 1971). Most adolescents have nondelinquent, or nonviolent, ways of achieving their goals and are effective at handling most social situations (Elliott, 1994). Furthermore, most adolescents are part of social networks of family members and peers and are committed to conventional norms and values which lower the potential for them to engage in violent or delinquent behavior. Under such circumstances, violent behavior would be irrational as it could cause negative ramifications (Elliott, 1994). However, when an adolescent lacks such bonds violent behavior may appear to be more rational. Many modern control theorists have reconoeptualized control theory as a partial explanation for delinquency or violence and have combined control theory with other theoretical 15 explanations, including social learning theory (Agnew, 1993; O'Donnell, Hawkins, & Abbott, 1995). W131 Bronfenbrenner' S (1979) theory of the ecology of human development states that in order to understand human development it is necessary to go beyond the immediate situation of the individual to consider the ecological environment as an expanding series of contexts that surround the individual's immediate setting (i.e. , microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem). It is the interrelationships between the environment, the individual’s behavior, and the social processes that are important to development. Ecological models of violence characterize violent actions as a result of individual, interpersonal, and social risk factors (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). Bronfenbrenner (1979), and others (e.g. , Tolan & Guerra, 1994b), described delinquency and violence as dependent on multiple influences across the biopsychosocial systems including the individual, family and peers (interpersonal relations), proximal social settings (e.g. , school, community), and broader social macrosystem influences (such as gun control). Ecological models assume that the multiple levels influencing an individual have direct effects on an individual's risk for delinquent or violent activity (Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). The ecologieal model has important implications for conflict resolution and violence prevention programs. According to this model, interventions must be designed to correspond to the biopsychosocial system which the program is intended to influence (Tolan & Guerra, 1994a; Zigler, et a1. , 1992). For example, school conflict l6 resolution programs often target the individual as the level of intervention (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b) while community-based intervention programs may target multiple systems for intervention (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). S . l I . I] Social learning theory describes the acquisition of behavior as a result of an interplay between an individual and his or her social experiences (environment). Bandura's (1977) Social Learning Theory describes how people use cognitive operations to interpret their social experiences and how these interpretations then come to influence their development and future behaviors. In social learning theory, learning occurs through instrumental conditioning (Winfree, Backstrém, & Mays, 1994). Accordingly, an individual is conditioned by positive and negative social experiences and mechanisms. Behavior, therefore, is acquired through the effects, outcomes, or consequences it has on a person's environment (Akers, 1985), and the resulting cognitive appraisals a person associates with the behavior. The primary processes by which conditioning is achieved are reinforcement and punishment. Behavior is said to be reinforced when repeated episodes are met with a response that influences the. actor to engage in the behavior again under similar circumstances. As a result, behavior increases. Behavior is punished when the response is such that the actor is discouraged from reengaging in the behavior under similar circumstances. As a result, the particular behavior decreases (Winfree et al. , 1994). The principles of reinforcement and punishment have been researched in l7 qualitative studies of gangs and gang members, and have been found to explain the process by which gang members regulate the behaviors of their peers (as described in Winfree et al., 1994). Individuals learn to evaluate behaviors through contact with others. Violent behavior is more likely to occur when adolescents develop, through reinforcement and punishment, orientations which are favorable to violence (Winfree et al. , 1994). The social interactions and networks of an individual provide the setting in which favorable (or unfavorable) behavior is defined. Specifically, social interactions and networks, through reinforcement of favorable behavior, and punishment of unfavorable behavior , promote a defined orientation towards behavior. In a school setting, an individual' 3 social interactions and networks usually consist of peers who hold similar values and ideas about behavior (Elliott, 1994; Luthar, 1995; Winfree et al., 1994). As a result, adolescents are likely to engage in behaviors similar to those of their friends (Luthar, 1995; Waegal, 1989). Associations with delinquent or violent peers, therefore, may increase the likelihood that an individual would engage in violent or delinquent behavior (Waegal, 1989). If violence is considered by a peer group to be a favorable response to conflict, individuals who are part of this group are more apt to resolve conflicts violently. The process of learning from a peer group (or other social networks) can occur vicariously, i.e. , by seeing similar others perform a behavior. Vicarious learning can result in behavior if an individual observes that the behavior is reinforced and cognitively appraises the behavior as positive (Bandura, 1982). Therefore, aggression 18 or violence is one of many responses available to an individual and can be learned through observation and imitation of aggressive or violent models. Subsequently, violence can be retained and maintained through social reinforcement. Along with social reinforcement or punishment, two additional cognitive processes, perceived self-efficacy and the expectations of the outcome of the behavior, are important determinants of an individual’s decision to act. These appraisals are the result of direct past experiences, vicarious or modeled experiences, verbal persuasions, and psychological states (Bandura, 1982). Aggressive and violent responses to interpersonal conflict can be reinforced when the actor: (1) has previous experiences (direct or vicarious) in which aggressive behavior led to positive results; (2) has positive perceptions of his or her capability to accomplish the perceived course of action needed to meet situational demands (self-efficacy) through aggressive behavior; (3) has lower perceived capabilities to accomplish the desired ends through alternative, nonviolent actions (social problem-solving competence); and (4) is in a social setting which does not reinforce the use of nonaggressive responses (outcome expectations). In sum, violence may occur in a Situation in which an actor lacks experience and/or perceived self-efficacy to generate multiple problem-solving approaches to resolve an interpersonal conflict and to evaluate their consequences. The components of social learning theory described above appear to underlie prominent models and training curricula that aim to foster the use of nonviolent conflict resolution techniques. Because social learning theory states that learning occurs 19 through the cognitive processes of observation, modeling, and response information, including the evaluation of outcomes and efficacy expectations (Bandura, 1977) , altering those components that previously reinforced violent conflict resolution techniques may reduce violence in interpersonal conflict situations. Therefore, a successful conflict resolution program should (1) create an environment that reinforces (through modeling and direct experience) nonviolent means of responding to interpersonal conflict, (2) foster the cognitive development of alternative social problem-solving and evaluation of consequences of action, (3) boost perceptions of self-efficacy for socially competent behavior, (4) improve the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior, and (5) increase the actual social competence for acting in specific interpersonal conflict Situations. Applying the concepts of social learning theory to conflict resolution programs, as prosocial conflict resolution techniques are shown to produce positive outcomes and to avoid punishing ones, and as individuals’ perceptions of personal efficacy are enhanced, prosocial conflict resolution techniques are likely to be retained and reinforced in social interactions. As a result, prosocial problem-solving competence in interpersonal conflict Situations can be increased. Programs based upon the principles of social learning theory have successfully promoted social problem-solving competence, enhanced perceptions of self-efficacy and outcome expectations, and reduced rates of high-risk behaviors (Allen, Leadbeater, & Aber, 1990; Caplan, Weissberg, Grober, Sivo, Grady, & Jacoby, 1992). Studies on the effects of these programs (Allen et al., 1990; Caplan et al. , 1992) have 20 demonstrated that social skills training programs can lead to the development of social competence, self—efficacy, and outcome expectations. They have shown, for example, the relationships between these constructs and positive growth in coping Skills and reductions in self-reported substance use. Because social skills training has successfully reduced other high-risk behaviors, it is relevant to consider and discuss these same constructs, i.e. , social problem-solving competence, perceptions of self-effieacy, and outcome expectations, in relation to reducing violent conflict resolution. The following sections include discussions of prosocial problem—solving competence, perceptions of self-efficacy in social conflict situations, and the expectations of the outcomes of socially competent behavior. Yeates and Selman (1989) described social problem-solving as a process that involves teaching students how to evaluate a situation, develop and implement a specific action, and evaluate the effects of their action. In a social problem-solving intervention, participants are trained to follow a sequence of steps when solving social problems (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). Social problem-solving interventions have been used effectively to alter the specific behaviors of targeted groups in both primary prevention programs and intervention efforts (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). A study conducted with 112 youth referred to a diagnostic center for treatment of antisocial behavior compared the effectiveness of three different intervention conditions. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions, social problem-solving skills training, social problem-solving skills training with in-vivo practice, or client- centered relationship therapy (Kazdin, Bass, Siege], & Thomas, 1989). Participants in 21 both social problem-solving conditions participated in 25 individual sessions designed to teach problem—solving Skills in generating alternative solutions, means-ends and consequential thinking skills, and perspective taking. Participants in the in-vivo condition also took part in related activities outside of the 25 sessions. Results indicated that participants in both of the problem-solving conditions showed higher prosocial and lower aggression scores (as evaluated by participant, teacher, and parent reports of behavior) one year after the intervention than did participants in the relationship therapy condition (Kazdin et al. , 1989). Scores for participants in the social problem-solving plus in-vivo condition were higher than scores for participants in the social problem-solving condition (Kazdin et al. , 1989). These results indicate that not only is social problem-solving a useful approach to enhancing prosocial behavior, but opportunities to practice new skills further strengthen program effectiveness. The effectiveness of social problem-solving programs may be due to their greater scope than other cognitive interventions and because they often include training in self- control, anger management, perspective taking, and attitude change (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). Multiple social problem-solving programs are being implemented in schools to reduce the prevalence of violent conflict resolution (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b). One such program is the WWW (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). The Commission on Violence and Youth (1993) included in its summary report that evaluations of school social problem-solving programs indicate that they can 22 "improve prosocial competence and reduce at-risk behavior among youth who are not seriously violence-prone by promoting nonviolent norms, lessening the opportunity for and elicitation of violent acts, and preventing the sporadic violence that emerges temporarily during adolescence" (p. 55). Reports of the impact of the We: WWW (Prothrow-Stith, 1987) and other conflict resolution programs indicate that participants show gains in targeted cognitive skills and beliefs, but most program evaluations have not included appropriate measures of behavior outcomes, and therefore, the effectiveness of such programs is uncertain (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b; Webster, 1993). Although empirically substantiated outcomes of violence prevention programs are few, research on the use of social problem-solving programs to alter specific behaviors lends support for the use of such programs to reduce violence. Systematic approaches to social problem-solving have been shown to foster social competence, problem-solving efficacy, and pro-social behavior (Caplan et al. , 1992; Yeates & Selman, 1989) in other targeted behavioral areas. Studies demonstrate that when students are taught effective, relevant solutions to interpersonal conflicts, they Show enhanced social competence and pro-social behaviors including the ability to use constructive techniques to de-escalate interpersonal conflicts (Johnson, Johnson, & Dudley, 1992; Shure & Spivak 1988; Yeates & Selman, 1989). Violence prevention and conflict resolution programs which incorporate these aspects may Show positive gains in the reduction of violence in interpersonal conflict situations. The following sections will discuss three of the primary elements of social leaming theory that have 23 been promoted through social problem-solving training programs, social problem- solving competence, perceived self-efficacy, and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior. WW Social competence, success at meeting particular age-appropriate societal expectations (Luthar, 1995), is a complex construct. Social competence is often defined in relation to success in solving interpersonal conflicts (Eisenberg & Harris, 1984) and in research has been operationalized as positive peer reputations, favorable ratings by teachers, and/or high grades in school (Luthar, 1995). Ford (1982) defined social competence as the ”attainment of relevant social goals in specified social contexts, using appropriate means, and resulting in positive developmental outcomes" (p. 323). As related to social learning theory, social competence includes an evaluation of consequences and action in a manner that increases the possibility of beneficial outcomes and limits the possibility of punishing outcomes. In terms of interpersonal conflict situations, socially competent behavior is behavior that allows for an effective resolution of an immediate conflict reducing the chances that an individual will face similar problems in the future. AS stated by the Commission on Violence and Youth (1993), an increase in prosocial competence is correlated with a reduction in at-risk violent behavior. In this current study, social competence refers to behaviors that embody both social skills and a prosocial orientation (Ford, 1982). Deficits in social competence have been linked to high-risk adolescent behaviors including substance abuse, school failure, delinquency (Caplan et a1. , 1992), aggressive 24 behavior (Pepler, King, & Byrd, 1991), and unprotected sexual intercourse (Allen et al. , 1990). Social problem-solving programs, aimed at promoting social competence in areas including impulsitivity, drug use, and aggression, have been shown to be successful at reducing the frequency of high-risk behaviors (Caplan et al. , 1992). These studies seem to indicate that social problem-solving competence may be a critical mediator in the relationship between problem—solving interventions and the reduction of high-risk behaviors. Social problem-solving programs, also referred to in the literature as social competence promotion programs (e.g. , Caplan et al. ,1992), are designed to enhance personal and interpersonal effectiveness and to prevent the development of maladaptive behavior, such as violence, drug use, and school failure. Social problem-solving programs impact social competence by teaching students developmentally appropriate skills and information. Such programs foster prosocial and health-enhancing values and beliefs, and create environmental supports to reinforce the real-life application of Skills (Weissberg, Caplan, & Sivo, 1989, as cited in Caplan et al., 1992). Such programs also act to reinforce or punish behavior through social mechanisms. Behavior is reinforced when other people respond in a way that causes the actor to engage in the behavior again under similar circumstances. Behavior is punished when the response from others is such that the actor is discouraged from engaging in the behavior again under similar circumstances (Winfree et al., 1994). This process is one way in which social problem-solving programs alter targeted behaviors. 25 In a study conducted by Caplan et al. (1992) relating social competence promotion to alcohol use by inner-city Sixth and seventh graders (72 program and 134 control students, 90% African-American), researchers found that students trained in social competence skills indicated gains in self-reported problem-solving efficacy, improved constructive conflict resolution skills with peers, and increased impulse control and popularity with peers. Furthermore, students trained in social competence did not show an increase in excessive alcohol use. However, self-report data from control I Students indicated that the number of times they reported drinking too much, the frequency of having three or more drinks, and the usual amount of alcohol they consumed on a single occasion increased (Caplan et al. , 1992). In the study by Caplan et al. (1992), social competence training consisted of a 20-session curriculum composed of lessons on stress management, self-esteem, problem-solving (including problem identification, alternative solution generation, evaluation of consequences, and plan implementation), information on substances and health, assertiveness, and social networks. Lessons were presented through didactic instruction, class discussion, videotapes, diaries, small-group role-plays, and work Sheets. Results indicated that training improved problem-solving efficacy for inner-city program students including enhanced social adjustment and interpersonal effectiveness. No gains were reported for the control students. The researchers concluded that the information and competence training may have equipped students with the critical thinking skills necessary to recognize the outcomes of their behaviors. Caplan et al. , also indieated that competence promotion, when applied to a Specific domain in a 26 developmentally appropriate content area, may be particularly instrumental in preventing psychosocial problems. Because behavior and perceptions are difficult to alter in a short period of time, researchers report being encouraged by the findings of this study. Empirical evidence from social problem-solving programs related to violence reduction is scarce (Tolan & Guerra, 1994b; Webster, 1993). Furthermore, social competence has not been directly investigated with respect to school violence prevention programs. However, given the findings that social competence training has been shown to be effective in improving the problem-solving skills and social behavior of trained adolescents in other domains, it is likely that social competence may act as a mediator between conflict resolution training and nonviolent conflict resolution. Furthermore, the results of research on social problem-solving programs demonstrate that training can foster the positive development of self-efficacy and outcome expectations of competent behavior. These findings suggest that social problem-solving approaches, such as conflict resolution training, may be effective in fostering the development of self-efficacy and positive outcome expectations of competent behavior. W As defined by Bandura (1977), an individual's self—efficacy expectation is one' s conviction in his or her ability to execute a behavior successfully to achieve a desired outcome. Self-efficacy is a contextual phenomenon that varies from situation to situation depending upon an individual's experience with similar situational demands and the individual's perceptions of his or her chances of success or failure (Bandura, 1977; Wheeler & Ladd, 1982). Beliefs about self-efficacy 27 are dependent upon an individual' S past experience of achievement in a domain, observation of what others are able to accomplish, attempts of others to mold feelings of self-efficacy through persuasion, and an individual' 8 own physiological State during a task (Bandura, 1982; Grusec, 1992). Self-efficacy theory been applied to research in a variety of domains, including academic achievement, health-related behavior, parenting styles, self-concept, and social behavior (Grusec, 1992). Self-efficacy has been shown to be an important mediator of behavior (Bandura, 1982). As described by Bandura (1982), skills and self-beliefs about efficacy are required for successful functioning in any activity. If perceptions of self-efficacy are low, individuals tend to behave ineffectively, even when they know what to do in the given situation (Bandura, 1982). Furthermore, individuals who perceive themselves as efficacious are more likely to persist in their efforts until they succeed (Bandura, 1982). As such, an individual who does not perceive him or herself as able to act socially competent in a given situation may decide to discontinue efforts in that Situation (Connolly, 1989; Wheeler & Ladd, 1982). Perceptions of self-efficacy influence an individual's choice of which behavior to initiate in a Situation. Beliefs about self-efficacy also influence the persistence and degree of effort exerted by an individual in trying to perform a behavior (Bandura, 1977; Lyman, Prentice-Dunn, Wilson, & Bonfilio, 1984). Perceived self—efficacy is highly related to a variety of social abilities including making friends, developing intimate bonds, being socially assertive, and participating in social groups (Connolly, 1989; Ford, 1982). 28 Caplan et al. (1992) found that along with fostering social competence, social problem-solving techniques influenced the development of adolescents' perceptions of self-efficacy. In other studies, the enhancement of perceived self-efficacy has led to the improvement of Skills or competencies (Grusec, 1992). Grusec stated that competencies and self-efficacy are acquired through experience, but in turn they also regulate the individual's experience in such a way that they are maintained. Research, such as that by Caplan et al. (1992) and Allen et al. (1990)., and described by Grusec, has suggested that there are strong relationships among self-efficacy, social competence, and Specific behaviors. WWW Along with one's perceptions of self-efficacy, one’s evaluations of the outcomes of behaviors may play a mediating role in how he or she chooses to respond to a specific Situation. Both self-efficacy and outcome expectations are potential key elements in understanding how adolescents act and perceive themselves in interpersonal interactions. Outcome expectations as defined by Bandura (1977) refer to an individual’ s estimate that a given performed behavior will lead to a desired outcome. While both perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations of specific behaviors have been related to social competence (Allen, Weissberg, & Hawkins, 1989), it is important to differentiate between the two (Bandura, 1977). An individual's belief in his or her ability to perform a given behavior (perceived self-efficacy) lacks meaning unless the individual also believes that performing the behavior will lead to desired outcome (outcome expectation). Furthermore, an individual may believe that a given outcome 29 could improve a situation but may feel unable to execute the behavior appropriately and may choose an alternative response. It may be that in order to enhance an adolescents' social competence, one must also foster a sense of self-efficacy and positive expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior. Allen et al. (1990) stated that perceptions of self—efficacy, when considered in reference to Specific situations and behaviors, have been related to a range of adaptive behaviors (Bandura, Adams, Hardy, & Howells, 1980; Allen et al., 1990). Existing research shows that past experiences influence an individual's perceptions of self-efficacy and outcome expectations. These findings provide evidence that prior positive experiences can improve perceptions of self-efficacy and outcome expectations and thereby also increase the degree of effort and persistence placed on performing competent behaviors (Allen et al. , 1990; Bandura, 1977). Therefore, low efficacy expectations and negative appraisals of behavioral outcomes can be extinguished by providing experiences that boost perceptions of self-efficacy and outcome expectations and disconfirm previous negative experiences (Bandura, 1977). Allen et al. (1990) found that adolescents' expectations influenced their self images in social Situations. Further, results indicated that adolescents' perceptions of self-efficacy are highly correlated to their social problem-solving competence. These researchers found that social problem-solving competence was negatively correlated with delinquency and drug use for both male and female research participants. Allen et al. (1990) suggested that past failures with competent behaviors (i.e. , prosocial behaviors) may lead to low perceptions of self-efficacy and to reduced 30 persistence in carrying out competent behaviors. They suggested that reduced persistence in carrying out competent behavior ean lead, over time, to further reductions in perceptions of self-efficacy until only less competent behaviors are perceived to be available to the adolescent (Allen et al. , 1990). Results of Allen et al. (1990) indicated the importance of considering adolescents' perceptions of self-efficacy and outcome expectations for competent behavior in designing and implementing a program aimed at promoting competent behavior in adolescents. Their study also suggested the need to allow for successful experiences in social conflict situations if adolescents are to gain positive perceptions of their self-efficacy and positive expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior. Furthermore, fostering self-efficacy and promoting positive expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior may be essential to the development of social competence and nonviolent conflict resolution strategies. Summant. Perceptions of self-efficacy and expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior are linked to adolescent social competence and may mediate the relationship between social problem-solving training, such as conflict resolution training, and social competence. Furthermore, social competence may mediate the relationship between adolescents' expectations and nonviolent interpersonal conflict resolution. If, through violence prevention training, adolescents are provided with the knowledge and opportunities for positive experiences and reinforcement for resolving interpersonal problem situations nonviolently, their perceptions of self-efficacy and 31 expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior may be enhanced. This enhancement, furthermore, may foster adolescents' social problem-solving competence, which in turn will promote nonviolent interpersonal conflict resolution strategies. A violence prevention training curriculum which may provide these opportunities for adolescents is the W (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). ll' 1 E . C . l E E l l The W15, developed by Prothrow-Stith (1987), is a program consisting of ten sessions designed to provide students with information on the risks of violence and homicide, to teach various alternatives to violence such as conflict resolution techniques, and to create a classroom and school environment that is nonviolent and that values violence prevention behavior (Prothrow—Stith et al., 1987). The Wm is didactic and cognitive and combines information delivery and situation role-plays. The curriculum stresses that while anger is normal, adolescents can be taught to change their responses to anger from violent or destructive to nonviolent or constructive. Through social problem-solving techniques, the curriculum attempts to teach students to be creative in their responses to anger and to determine for themselves the risks and benefits of fighting. Fighting is presented as one choice among many to resolve conflict. The content of the ten-session Violence WW (Prothrow-Stith, 1987) is described in more detail in the following chapter (Methods). 32 The W is based upon the principles of social learning theory. As described earlier, in social learning theory, behavior (direct or modeled) is reinforced when it is met with a response that influences the actor to engage in the behavior again under similar circumstances. AS a result, perceptions of self—effieacy in social situations increase, expectations of the outcomes of competent (prosocial) behavior increase, and behavior increases. Behavior is punished when the actor is discouraged from reengaging in the behavior under similar circumstances. AS a result of punishment, the particular behavior decreases (Winfree et al. , 1994). It is through this process of reinforcement and modeling of behavior that conflict resolution programs, such as the WW attempt to increase students’ use of nonviolent conflict resolution techniques. The program attempts to provide adolescents with effective prosocial means of resolving conflict though information provision and modeling of specific behaviors. Role play situations are used frequently during training sessions to allow for the acting out and modeling of appropriate ways to resolve conflicts nonviolently. Students can learn vicariously of the reinforcement and reward of one of their peers in such a situation. These behaviors are then reinforced through discussions in the classroom that provide social contexts and interactions in which students can evaluate the behavior. Furthermore, students are encouraged to talk about conflicts they have seen or experienced during the previous week. This interaction allows for direct feedback (reinforcement or punishment) from other students and from the program facilitator to 33 the person who was involved in the conflict and also provides vicarious learning experiences to other students in the class. The W was first implemented in several Boston high schools in 1984. It has since gained acceptance throughout the nation and has been implemented in various other cities. The curriculum has also been used as part of a larger community-wide intervention in Boston that included a mass media campaign and the involvement of community agencies (Hausman, Spivack, Prothrow-Stith, Roeber, 1992). Results of school evaluations of the WW Adolescents have been mixed. A study conducted in 1987 showed significant gains in students' knowledge about violence. School suspension data suggested that suspensions due to violence were reduced (Prothrow-Stith, et al. , 1987). However, a study conducted by Spiro, et al., (1989) and discussed in Webster (1993) of tenth-graders at six inner-city schools around the U.S. reported that no significant changes were found in students' total post-test knowledge about violence; their attitudes about ways to handle conflicts; acceptance of violence; violence locus of control; self-esteem; or self-reported fighting, drug use, or weapon carrying. However, when stratified by school and compared to the control group, the treatment group in one school had greater gains in knowledge about violence, and the treatment group in another school demonstrated greater gains in self-esteem. In twoschools, results indicated that students were less likely to believe that people other than themselves were responsible for preventing fights (Webster, 1993). Researchers attributed the apparently contradictory 34 results to the stylistic differences of the individual program implementers. Furthermore, researchers attributed the overall lack of program effectiveness to targeting the program at high school students and not at the preferred middle school population. Despite inconclusive results, the WWW (Prothrow-Stith, 1987) has received highly positive anecdotal results and continues to be one of the most widely implemented and promising conflict resolution programs in the country (DeJong, 1994; Webster, 1993). The rate at which schools have adopted programs and the rate at which programs are being funded are both increasing rapidly. While programs may be inexpensive to implement at an individual school or school system, when considered in aggregate, the program costs add up to considerable personnel and monetary resources (Webster, 1993). In consideration of these efforts and costs, programs need to be closely evaluated to determine the effects they have on our schools and students. To date, there have been few empirical evaluations conducted on programs developed to reduce violence in schools (Commission on Violence and Youth, 1993; Elliott, 1994; Johnson et al., 1994; Johnson, Johnson, & Stevahn, 1995). During the 1993-1994 school year a county office on violence reduction in conjunction with a large, metropolitan public school district in the Midwest implemented a dispute management training program based upon the Meme WWW (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). This current study is an investigation of the effects of this program. 35 Goals and Hypotheses The current investigation was exploratory research on the effects of a dispute management program. The goals were to determine the effects of the intervention on adolescents' social problem-solving competence, perceptions of self-efficacy for performing competent behaviors, and expectations about the outcomes of competent behavior in interpersonal conflict situations. This investigation also attempted to determine the relationship between social problem-solving competence, perceptions of self-efficacy, outcome expectations for competent behavior, and nonviolent conflict resolution. It was eXpected that because the WWW provided students with knowledge and opportunities for positive experiences in resolving interpersonal problem situations nonviolently, it would foster the positive development of adolescents' self-efficacy and outcome expectations for competent behavior in interpersonal conflict situations. As a result, adolescents’ prosocial problem-solving competence would also be enhanced and would mediate the increased use of nonviolent conflict resolution strategies. It was hypothesized that students who participated in the dispute management training would: (1) perceive themselves as more efficacious, (2) have more positive expectations for the outcomes of competent behavior, (3) report more socially competent responses to interpersonal conflict Situations, and (4) resolve interpersonal conflict situations nonviolently more frequently than students who did not participate in the program. 36 A model was developed to test the effects of the dispute management program (see Figure 1). In the model, perceived self-efficacy and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior were hypothesized to mediate the relationship between dispute management training and social problem-solving competence. In addition, social problem-solving competence was hypothesized to mediate the relationship between perceived self-efficacy and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior. 37 ooze—08M 8580 32228 Z No. 0833800 328-335 38.0. k ac. an. 80388me 080350 seamsum 328qu EB: Emacs s ”Ema co. means Deoaowmomz 83me Chapter Two METHOD Setting A comprehensive violence prevention program was initiated by a Midwestem metropolitan County Office on Violence Reduction in conjunction with its public school system. As part of this program, seventh grade homerooms at two middle schools were chosen to participate in dispute management training for a ten-week period starting the end of March, 1994. Homeroom classes were chosen to participate by each school's administration in conjunction with the homeroom teacher. With the exception of a homeroom of honor students at one school, and a classroom of students with learning disabilities in another school, students were assigned to homeroom classrooms to reflect a cross-section of academic ability. Students at all levels of academic ability participated in the dispute management training. Approximately 100 students per school participated in the training. The County Office on Violence Reduction and the public school system chose the schools receiving training from approximately 33 schools with sixth, seventh, and eighth grades. They also selected two middle schools with similar demographics, student population, and rates and patterns of violent or illegal acts to be comparison schools. Information on the incidence and type of violence was obtained from a central computerized data system maintained by the public school system for reporting violations of the school offense code. 38 39 In the school system’s records, offenses are reported individually and are included in one of the following three categories: type "A" offenses include those that would be considered criminal or delinquent acts such as fighting, possession of a weapon, and breaking and entering; type "B" offenses include non—criminal type activities such as truancy; and type ”V” offenses are violent offenses such as use of a firearm or hitting a teacher. A review of the Student Code Violations report for the 1992 - 1993 school year indicated that 1610 students attended the two treatment schools combined. At these two schools, seven violent (type V) acts and 197 illegal acts (type A) were reported in the Student Code Violations report. The report indicated that 1197 students attended the two control schools combined. Reported violations in the control schools indicated that four violent acts and 122 illegal acts were committed. A chi-square test for independence indicated that there were no significant differences between the number of students who were violent or who committed illegal acts in the treatment and control schools, X2 (1, N = 2,807) = 3.03, NS. The results of the chi-squared analysis indicated that the the treatment and control schools were statistically similar in terms of the number and type of violent acts. The violence reduction project was designed to take place in the following three stages: (1) dispute management training for students, (2) peer mediation, and (3) teacher dispute mediation training. The long term goal of the County Office on Volence Reduction is to establish ongoing dispute management programs in all of the middle schools in the metropolitan public school system and throughout the rest of county. These plans would include providing training opportunities in the two 40 comparison middle schools that participated in this study. An evaluation of the entire program was conducted by investigators associated with the Institute for Public Policy and Social Research at Michigan State University. The overall evaluation addressed the following four questions: (1) Have students learned non-violent methods of conflict resolution? How are these techniques utilized? (2) What is the change in the students' feeling of safety at school? (3) Has the climate of the school been altered to support the intervention? (4) What is the impact of the intervention on the numbers and types of school behavioral violations? The student population in the public school system used in this study is approximately 95 % African-American. The system-wide teacher and staff population is approximately 60 to 70% African-American. The student population in the schools participating in this study was close to 100% African-American. The student to teacher ratio at each of the schools was approximately 25 to 30 students to one teacher. Subjects At the conclusion of dispute management training at the participating schools, seventh graders at the two treatment and two comparison schools were recruited to be interviewed. Researchers had a list of students from each of the seventh grade homeroom classrooms at each school. In each of the treatment schools, twenty-five of the approximately 100 seventh grade students who participated in the dispute management training were randomly selected to be interviewed. Twenty-five students in homeroom classrooms that did not receive training were also selected at random to be interviewed. 41 In each treatment and control school a member of the school' 8 staff was given a list of the students who were selected at random. This staff member managed the process of recruiting students and delivering them to the interview site. At the beginning of each interview students were informed that their participation was voluntary. In addition, students were told that they could stop the interview at any time. While some students may have chosen not to answer specific questions, no student refused to participate. Two-hundred and eleven interviews were completed at the four schools. At the completion of the interview at the treatment schools, students were asked to identify whether they had participated in the conflict resolution training. In one treatment school twenty-four students interviewed identified themselves as having participated in training; in the other treatment school fourteen students responded that they had been trained. A total of 38 Students interviewed indicated that they had been trained in dispute management skills. Sixty-five students interviewed at the treatment schools identified themselves as not participating in the conflict resolution training program. Table 1 indicates that the numbers of students who identified themselves as being trained in dispute management skills is far less than the number of students who gave a homeroom number that corresponded with homerooms receiving training. According to homeroom numbers reported by students during interviews, 67 students in the treatment schools reported that they were assigned to homerooms that received training. The discrepancy between self-report data and data of students in Specific homerooms may indicate that students were not aware of the kind of program in which 42 Table 1 I"! ‘ :-.,.-.;£.- n”..- '. e'a. _- U .. ... -- --l-.,.,.. a. . ... L. W _Sel£-rencn_ W School Condition Treatment Trained 38 67 Not trained 65 36 Control Not applicable 108 43 they were involved. Because of the discrepancy, it is difficult to know the true number of interviewed students who participated in training. It was decided to use the count obtained through self-report in all analyses, that is, 38 students. At each of the control schools a staff member was given a list of 50 seventh grade students selected at random. Fifty-two students at one control school were interviewed, and 56 at the other control school were interviewed. A total of 108 Students at the control schools participated in the interview process. Design Student interviews were used to collect information about the relationships among dispute management training, social problem-solving competence, perceived social self-efficacy in performing competent behavior, and the expectations of the outcome of competent behavior. In addition, interviews included questions on self-reported pro- and anti-social behaviors, including the use of nonviolent conflict resolution strategies. The current study was an independent groups, posttest-only investigation of the effects of dispute management training on middle school students. Students trained in dispute management were compared to students who attended the same school but were not trained, and to students who attended a control school who also did not receive training in conflict resolution skills. Schools were selected due to their similar Size, student population, demographics, and number and type of violent incidents reported in school code violations (as described previously). Matching schools for these characteristics provided similar student populations that could be considered equivalent in nature at the onset of the 44 dispute management intervention. Furthermore, the results of chi-square analysis of data included in the School Violations Report indicated that there were no Significant differences in the frequencies of violent and illegal acts between the treatment and control schools. This investigation, therefore, was well-suited for a post-only measurement design. The dispute management training program was the independent variable in this study. Social problem-solving competence, perceived self-efficacy for performing competent behaviors, outcome expectations of competent behavior, and self-reported pro- and anti-social conflict resolution behaviors were the dependent variables. Program Description The dispute management training program was delivered to students in the form of weekly one-hour sessions for a period of ten weeks. Two independent consultants were hired by the County Office on Violence Reduction to teach the dispute management training in the two treatment schools. The curriculum used in the training, the W8, (Prothrow-Stith, 1987), was designed to stress that anger is a normal emotion experienced by all people. Furthermore, the curriculum recognized that aggression and violence are reactions to anger and conflict that are learned through interaction with other people. The curriculum aimed to teach students how to alter their reactions to interpersonal conflict through lessons on self- control, anger management, perspective taking, and attitude change. Most of the sessions included role play situations and frank conversations about interpersonal conflicts students experienced from one session to the next. The role 45 plays and discussions allowed students the opportunities to learn new responses to interpersonal conflict from their peers and from the facilitator. More specifically, the use of role play situations provided students the opportunity to learn how interpersonal conflict can escalate into violence and the possible avenues for diverting the conflict away from a violent conclusion. Students who acted in the role plays had the opportunity to learn through direct action, and students who watched the role plays had the opportunity to learn vicariously through the modeled behavior. Furthermore, all students were witnesses to reinforcement (or punishment) from student and facilitator feedback on the resolution of conflict portrayed in the role plays. Discussions of conflicts students experienced from one week to the next allowed for direct and vicarious learning and feedback from students and the facilitator. The following paragraphs provide a brief overview of the sessions included in the W W (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). Session One of the Wis, “There is a Lot of Violence in Society," aims to determine what information and misinformation students have about violence. Topics discussed during Session One include the causes and effects of violence and the extent and types of violence in society. The overall focus of the Wm (Prothrow-Stith, 1987) is interpersonal violence. The curriculum defines interpersonal violence as accidental or intentional violence that occurs between two or more persons. Intentional violence, as defined in Session One, includes stranger violence, that is, violence between persons who do not know each other, as might occur incidentally during the commission of a crime; sexual 46 violence, such as rape; and acquaintance violence, that is, violence among people who know each other, such as friends, peers, or family members (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). Session Two, “Homicide: Statistics and Characteristics," presents statistical data on homicide and the characteristics of homicide. Students are challenged to think about the statistical information and the relationships between homicide and weapons, alcohol, and arguments. Homicide is presented as the second leading cause of death for young people ages fifteen to twenty-four (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). Session Three, “Exploring Risk Factors,” presents violence-related risk factors introduced during Session Two, and describes the physiological effects of alcohol on the brain and alcohol’s relevance to interpersonal violence. Information on weapons and homicide is presented in detail. In addition, the curriculum provides facilitators with information suitable for a mini-lecture on poverty, race, and homicide. In sum, the information presents poverty as another factor that contributes to honricide and as a more salient factor than race. Session Four, "Anger is Normal," describes anger as a normal and natural part of life. Information is provided on the physiological changes that occur when someone is angry, including the concept of ”fight or flight” during an interpersonal confrontation. Students are encouraged to talk about what makes them angry. Session Five, ”There are Healthy and Unhealthy Ways to Express Anger, " builds on the idea presented in Session Four that anger is normal and illustrates the healthy and unhealthy ways to express anger. The session aims to help students understand that there are constructive and destructive ways to deal with anger. 47 Session Six, "There's More to Lose than to Gain from Fighting," challenges Students to compare the positive and negative consequences of fighting. The session attempts to demonstrate that the negative consequences of fighting outweigh the positive. To do so, students are asked to make a list of the positive consequences of fighting (e.g. , winning, proving a point) and a list of the negative consequences (e. g. , being hurt, being embarrassed.) Session Seven, ”What Happens Before, During, and After a Fight,” illustrates the steps that precede many fights situations. Included in this section is a discussion of the roles of peer pressure, increased emotions, and nonverbal indicators in a fight situation. Students create role plays of fight situations and are asked to dissect the scenes for cues to how the fights escalated, how emotions were expressed verbally and nonverbally, and the role of peer pressure in the fights (Prothrow-Stith, 1987). Session Eight, “Preventing Violence, " helps students determine ways that violence may be prevented through analyzing a fight. In addition, students discuss the differences between prevention and intervention in a fight situation, and outline methods to prevent violence in a school environment. Session Nine, "Fighting - What Else is There, " emphasized that there are many choices available to students other than fight or flight when confronted with a conflict. Students are asked to identify obstacles to nonviolent resolutions of conflict, and to describe how violence is glamorized in our society by television, etc. The curriculum includes multiple situations that could lead to conflict and provides students the 48 opportunity to identify the choices available to try to resolve the situations nonviolently. Session Ten, "Practice Throwing a Curve, " provides students with an opportunity to practice the Skills of nonviolent conflict resolution. This final session encourages students to be empathetic with their opponents and to identify alternatives to the fight or flight concept discussed in previous sessions. Role play situations are again encouraged. Students are challenged to summarize the perspectives of both parties in the conflict situations, to use the Skills from the program in a role-play situation, and to recognize that fighting is only one of the several choices in a conflict situation. Procedure Student interviews were conducted in the auditoriums, libraries, or cafeterias of each school during times when the rooms were not occupied by other students. Interviews were conducted in these areas to allow students and interviewers adequate privacy and quiet space. Student interviews were conducted by graduate students from the Criminal Justice and Psychology departments at Michigan State University and by local residents who were familiar with the public school system. Interviewers were trained by the investigators in interviewing techniques and were familiar with the instruments prior to conducting their first interview. AS much as possible, interviewers reflected the ethnic background of the student population in the schools. Each interview participant was interviewed for approximately thirty nrinutes during school hours. Interviewers were asked to write verbatim each response given by the 49 students for each scenario and to circle the response students indicated for the number of times they had engaged in the activities included in the different items on self-report delinquency, victimization, and positive conflict resolution techniques. In exchange for the cooperation of the school adrrrinistration and the students' participation, each school received five dollars per student interviewed. As part of an agreement between the county office of violence reduction and the public school system, individual guardian permission for students to be interviewed was not required. Student participants were made aware that their responses were confidential and were reminded that they should decline to answer any questions that they felt uncomfortable answering honestly. Measures Revised scenarios from the Adolescent Problem Inventory (API) for boys (Freedman, Rosenthal, Donohoe, Schlundt, & McFall, 1978) and the Problem Inventory for Adolescent Girls (PIAG) (Gaffney & McFall, 1981) and scenarios developed for this study were used to measure adolescents’ social problem-solving competence, expectations of self-efficacy in performing competent behaviors, and outcome expectations for competent behavior in interpersonal conflict Situations (procedure is similar to that used by Allen et al., 1990) (see Appendix A). A total of seven scenarios were used, three adapted from the API or PIAG and four created for this investigation (see Table 2). The interpersonal conflict scenarios included social conflicts or potential conflicts with parent, teachers, and peers in Situations that an adolescent may experience directly or may be able to imagine Table 2 C [51.5.2151 Scenario Scenario one Scenario two Scenario three Scenario four Scenario five Scenario Six Scenario seven 50 Scenario Content You’ve been disrupting a substitute teacher's class all week, and She sends you up to the principal' s office again. The principal meets you at the door and says, "You have been sent here three times this week and I'm excluding you this time!" Your mother has been nagging you about getting home immediately after school. One day you get home an hour late and your mother yells at you, ”Do you know what time it is? Where have you been?” A kid you know is a drug dealer. He found out that school security is going to check his locker after the next class. He asks you to keep his stuff in your locker. You think that one of your classmates has recently been Spreading nasty rumors about you. You don't know this student well, so you don't know why she (he) is saying these things. You're upset about these rumors, and you want the rumors to stop. You are walking home alone down a dark street. Two boys (girls) are following you. You don't know them, but they are about your age. They catch up with you and say, "We want your jacket. " You are Sitting with your friends in the lunch room. A girl (boy) you don't know spills her (his) drink on you. One of your friends tells her (him) to apologize, but she (he) refuses. You are at school and you see another student talking to your girlfriend (boyfriend). They are laughing and smiling and seem to be having fun. Suddenly, your girlfriend (boyfriend) looks at you and then turns quickly away. 51 experiencing. The content of the scenarios was reviewed by school officials including teachers and principals, students, and psychologists who agreed that the Situations were similar to those a student may experience. Adolescents' self-reported responses to the conflict Situations presented during the student interview were rated to measure social problem-solving competence. Students were asked to consider what they would have done in the conflict situation in comparison to a sample response of how another adolescent responded in the same situation. Perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations of competent behavior were obtained by rating these responses. Social problem-solving competence and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior were rated using coding manuals developed for this study. Perceived self-efficacy was rated on a Likert—type scale. In the next section, background information related to the development of the coding manuals for social problem-solving competence and the expectations of the outcome of competent behavior is discussed. The sections following present information on the measurement of the constructs social problem-solving competence, perceived self-efficacy, and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior. Finally, nonviolent conflict resolution behavior is discussed. Dcxclapmmmtmmmnuals Two undergraduate psychology majors seeking independent research credits worked with the principal researcher to develop a coding manual for social problem- solving competence for the six interpersonal conflict scenarios and to code the interview information according to the developed manuals (see Appendix A). The 52 three coders met on a weekly basis from May to August, 1994. One of the undergraduate students continued with the project to assist in developing a coding manual for outcome expectations for the six scenarios (see Appendix B). The development of the coding manual for outcome expectations and the coding process was accomplished from September to December, 1994. Each undergraduate student received two independent research credits for each semester of her participation. Responses collected from the first wave of data collection (used in the overall evaluation project) were used as the basis for the development of the coding manuals. The process used to develop the coding manuals ensured that all responses included in the coding manuals are ones that were actually given by students asked to respond to the scenarios. Of the original seven scenarios presented to students, six scenarios were coded using the developed manuals. The remaining scenario (Scenario One as indicated in Table 2) was discarded because initial review of students' responses indicated that there was no distribution of answers. Instead, the majority of students gave the same or a very Similar response. Student interviews were organized by a four digit student number which indicated the school the student attended (one through four), the wave of data collection (wave two of the overall evaluation), and their interview number. 5 . l E l l -S l . [2 Social problem-solving competence is defined as a response that effectively resolves the interpersonal conflict situation at hand and makes it less likely that the subject will experience more problems of this type in the future. As defined by Ford 53 (1982), and applied to this investigation, social competence embodies both social skills and a pro-social orientation. To measure social problem-solving competence, each student was presented with the interpersonal conflict scenarios one at a time and was asked to consider his or her response to the Situation. Students were asked to answer the question, ”What would you say or do now?” Student responses were written in the interview booklet and were used to code social problem-solving competence. The manual developed to code social problem-solving competence is similar to a manual used by Allen et al. (1990). In the manual, social competence is rated on a zero to eight scale with a score of zero corresponding to a very incompetent or very ineffective response, and a score of eight corresponding to a very competent or very effective response. Maximally competent responses are those which were identified by school administrators and teachers as most likely to resolve the conflict and reduce the chance that the adolescent would experience a Similar situation in the future. The midpoint on this scale is a four which corresponds to a response that is neither competent nor incompetent, i.e. , that neither directly helps nor hurts the Situation or individual involved in the interpersonal conflict. Responses to the scenarios gained from the first wave of data collection (used in the overall project evaluation) were reviewed by each coder who used them to create a preliminary ranking of responses using the zero to eight scale. Coders worked on one scenario at a time and reviewed and discussed preliminary coding schemes for each scenario at weekly meetings. Meetings provided the opportunity for the coders to discuss openly their reasons for placing each response at each level on the scale. This 54 process initially led to the development of a category for each scenario at all nine levels. As more responses were reviewed, the coding manual was revised to reflect more accurately the responses given and the level of social problem-solving competence represented by the response. levels within scenarios were combined and collapsed if the coders evaluated that two categories represented the same level of social competence. Collapsing levels within scenarios in this manner led to the development of scales with different coding levels per scenario. For example, if a scenario had a level six that was determined to represent the same level of social competence as responses originally coded as level five, the two category levels were combined. Because this on-going process led to each scenario having different possible coding levels, the final scores for social problem-solving competence were standardized using the z-statistic procedure. In the beginning of the coding process, all coders rated the same interviews starting with groups of ten interviews. Any discrepancies on scenario coding levels were discussed at the weekly meetings. Each coder presented her argument for why she coded the response at a given level. Once interrater reliability was established at over 90% across scenarios for the ten selected interviews, the three coders rated all six scenarios for all student interviews. Percent agreement interrater reliabilities for coding all of the interviews were calculated for each scenario and ranged from 89.6 to 97.2 percent (see Table 3). The primary researcher assigned the final coding score to the scenarios in which the three coders were discrepant. In the end, each student interview had codes for each scenario response given. Responses such as ”no answer” 55 Table 3 EH]. 5 !'§ § .12“ 25.1. E I ResrrltL Scenario Mean Standard Corrected Item Deviation Total Correlation Scenario 2 -3.17 1.49 .18 Scenario 3 -.19 .69 .23 Scenario 4 .34 .88 .23 Scenario 5 .46 .91 .33 Scenario 6 -.29 1.07 .14 a = .43 Percent agreement interrater reliability range 89.6 to 97 .2 Table 4 {‘W-Is at. ° 0 .I.‘-. .IH o 1")..011'0 0||'.'.‘1:.‘l-0 Results Scenario Mean Standard Corrected Item Deviation Total Correlation Scenario 3 3.04 1.62 .26 Scenario 4 3.05 1.49 .45 Scenario 5 3.03 1.53 .36 Scenario 6 3.09 1.25 .27 Scenario 7 3.64 1.59 .35 a = .58 Percent agreement interrater reliability range 88.6 to 94.8 Table 5 E l. H. 3 l . E E . l§ lE‘EEE Results Scenario Mean Standard Corrected Item Deviation Total Correlation Scenario 2 4.56 .88 .19 Scenario 4 4.45 .83 .27 Scenario 5 3.97 1.49 .29 Scenario 6 3.35 1.47 .25 Scenario 7 3.86 1.47 .25 56 were excluded as missing data when analyses were preformed. The response, "I don't know, " corresponded to a zero or a very ineffective response. A response of "nothing" was coded at different level for each scenario because in some scenarios doing nothing is an effective response to the situation while in others it is not. Scale reliability analysis was calculated for social problem-solving Competence scores on the six scenarios (a = .41). A review of the corrected item-total correlations indicated that Scenario Seven (as indicated on Table 3) was not highly correlated with the other five scenarios. This scenario was discarded for social problem-solving competence. Reliability analysis was run on the remaining five scenarios ((1 = .42). An average social problem-solving scale score was created to account for any missing data in the remaining five scenarios. A total of 204 student interviews were included in the coding process and the final social competence scale; the remaining seven student interviews represent missing data. Missing data may be the result of students declining to answer Specific questions. The average scale score was used for all subsequent analyses. QumeExpectations As defined by Bandura (1977), outcome expectations refer to an individual's perception of the liklihood that a given performed behavior will lead to a desired outcome. Adolescents' expectations about the outcomes of competent behavior were obtained by presenting the students with the seven interpersonal conflict situations a second time, followed by a competent hypothetical response of another adolescent to the conflict situation (similar to the process used by Allen et al. , 1990). Students were 57 asked to answer the open-ended question, "If you tried this, what do you think would happen?” The response of the hypothetical adolescent was derived from competent responses to the scenarios as evaluated by school officials and psychologists as most likely to resolve the conflict at hand and to reduce the chance that the adolescent would experience a similar situation in the future. The process for developing the coding manual for outcome expectations was Similar to the process described above for social problem-solving competence. Responses designating different levels were again derived from the responses collected during wave one interviews. Outcome expectations were rated on a one to five scale, with one corresponding to an expectation of a very negative outcome, and five corresponding to an expectation of a very positive outcome. Each scenario had a response category at each of the five possible levels. The two coders working on the manual initially worked independently to determine the placement of possible responses on the coding scale and met weekly to discuss response levels. All responses were coded by each coder to assure interrater reliability. Percent agreement interrater reliabilities were calculated and ranged from 88.6 to 94.8% (see Table 4). Scale reliability analysis was conducted and the initial scale alpha for the Six scenarios was .53. The corrected item-total correlations indicated that Scenario Two (as indicated in Table 4) was not highly related to the other scenarios. Scenario Two was removed from the scale producing a more reliable scale of five scenarios (a = .5 8). To account for missing data, an average scale score was computed using the remaining five scenarios. A total of 138 student interviews were included in the 58 coding process; the remaining 73 student interviews represent missing data. Missing data may be the result of students declining to answer Specific questions. The average scale score was used in the subsequent analyses. The validity of the two coding manuals was tested using a group of graduate students in psychology and psychology professors as independent judges. Each judge was presented with a scenario and with the possible responses to that scenario, i.e. the responses collected during wave one of interviews. Judges were asked to rank order the responses for social problem-solving competence and outcome expectations according to the definitions used in the coding manuals. As such, judges were asked to order the social problem-solving competence responses from zero to eight, with a zero corresponding to a very incompetent or very ineffective response, and a score of eight corresponding to a very competent or very effective response. Responses related to the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior were rank ordered from one to five with a one corresponding to an expectation of a very negative outcome and a five corresponding to an expectation of a very positive outcome. Judges ordered the responses in the same order as was decided by the original coders. This exercise provided a check on the ordering system used by the original coders. Welt-Efficacy As defined by Bandura (1977), an individual's self-efficacy expectation is one's conviction in his or her ability to execute a behavior successfully to achieve a desired outcome. To evaluate social self-efficacy in performing competent behaviors, adolescents were asked to consider the hypothetical response of the other teenager and 59 to respond to the question, "Do you think you could do what he (she) did if you tried?” Adolescents were asked to rate themselves on a Likert-type scale ranging from zero to five where a score of zero corresponded to ”definitely could not do it, " and five corresponded to ”definitely could do it. " Scale reliability analysis was run on self-efficacy for the six scenarios (a = .44). A review of the corrected item-total correlations indieated that Scenario Three (see Table 5) was not highly related to the other five scenarios and was therefore removed from the scale. Seale reliability analysis was conducted on the remaining five scenarios ((1 = 46). An average perceived self—efficacy scale score was computed to account for any missing data in the five remaining scenarios. A total of 209 student interviews were included in the coding process; the remaining two student interviews represent missing data. Missing data may have resulted from students declining to answer Specific questions. The average scale score was used for all subsequent analyses. 11 . l [2 H' E l . Students were asked to respond to a series of ten questions aimed at deterrrrining the number and kinds of interpersonal conflicts and antisocial behavior they have been involved in during the ten week period prior to being interviewed. The ten week time period corresponds to the ten weeks of dispute management training at the two participation schools. Students were asked to indicate how many times Since winter break (ten weeks prior to being interviewed) they had: (1) been in a fist fight, (2) talked their way out of a fight, (3) messed up school property, (4) been sent to the principal's office for bad behavior, (5) carried a gun/knife to school, (6) stopped 60 people from fighting each other, (7) threatened to hurt someone, (8) been suspended or excluded from school, (9) been physically hurt by someone at school, and (10) had someone take something from them using physical force. These items included questions on self-report delinquency, victimization, and positive conflict resolution techniques. Reliability analysis run on the ten items indicated that five of the items (1 , 3, 4, 7, 8) were highly related to each other (a = .74) (see Table 6). The direction of items was reversed as necessary. An average score for these five items was computed to create a nonviolent scale score for each interviewee. This scale was used in the hierarchical regression analyses discussed in the following chapter. 61 O . I O O . O C out: I 0 '.HI \.-..'.. .0 Hi -Ha1l.0.- -.‘I.Ao - tier. Question Since winter break (beginning of March), how many times have you: 1. Been in a fist fight 3. Messed up school property 4. Been sent to the principal' s office for bad behavior 7. Threatened to hurt someone 8. Been suspended or excluded from school a = .74 Chapter Three RESULTS A series of analyses of variance was conducted to investigate the effect of dispute management training on adolescents' social problem-solving competence, perceived self-efficacy in social conflict situations, expectations of the outcome of competent behavior, and nonviolent conflict resolution. Scheffé post hoc multiple comparison tests were used to examine group differences further. A series of hierarchical multiple regressions was used to analyze the proposed model predicting nonviolent conflict resolution from dispute management training, perceived self-efficacy, expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior, and social problem-solving competence. The following results are presented in order of hypothesis. Analysis of Variance Hymthesisflne Dispute management training will foster the positive development of adolescents' problem-solving competence in interpersonal conflict Situations. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted using experimental condition (i.e. , students trained in dispute management skills, students who attended an experimental school who were not trained in dispute management skills, and students who attended a control school) as the independent variable and average social problem- solving competence as the dependent variable. This AN OVA was Significant (E (2, 201) = 4.23, p < .05; n2 = .04) (see Table 7). Scheffé post hoc multiple comparison procedure indicated that the mean social problem-solving competence scale score for 62 63 82 H ZV Ge" 5 8m" 7Q cove—80M 8°. 3v 8a was 8. 8d 8. a; S. ”an 688022382 Size 8qu £qu 2858?.— So. m2 3 a. 3. ea 8; can R. NS“ 283.5 serve @qu amaze 8o. mz 8n 6. P. as 8. 8.... as. o3. seem—sum 38m @2qu gaze 5qu 3. 3v 8m m3. 3. 8.- mm. 8.- me. an. 88358 38m ad a \e ouch em 2 am 2 on 2 Season 8:58: 628 8.280 32.8 geostaxm 358,—. Dogmeou ....o. a ..4 .. ..«... .o-..o x..:.T.... ...- 9.... “.0 ....‘..1... ...x....... -:... a. 3 I -. .r... ...-........ ..‘ . I... 13‘. .r... . ..m. 7&5... A. 035. 64 students trained in dispute management skills (M = .22, SD = .48) was significantly higher than either group of students not trained in dispute management skills. There were no significant differences between the mean social problem-solving competence sale scores for students attending an experimental school who were not trained in dispute management skills (M = -.08, SD = .55) and students attending a control school (M = -.03, SD = .51) (see Table 7). W Dispute management training will foster the positive development of adolescents' self-efficacy for performing competent behaviors in interpersonal conflict situations. A one-way analysis of variance was conducted using experimental condition as the independent variable and perceived self-efficacy as the dependent variable. Results, reported in Table 7, indicated that there were no significant differences between experimental groups with respect to perceived self-efficacy scale scores (E (2, 206) = .61, NS; 112 = .006). Hmhesiflhmc Dispute management training will foster the development of adolescents' expectations about the positive outcomes of performing competent behavior in interpersonal conflict situations. A one-way AN OVA was utilized to examine the relationship between experimental condition as the independent variable and outcome expectations as i the dependent variable. The analysis, reported in Table 7, indicated that dispute management training 65 was not significantly related to adolescents' expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior (E (2, 135) = .12, NS; 112 = .002). * Hierarchical Regression Model Predicting Social Problem-Solving Competence W There is a relationship between dispute management training, social problem- solving competence, perceptions of self-efficacy, outcome expectations for competent behavior, and nonviolent conflict resolution. A model, depicted in Figure l, to predict nonviolent conflict resolution was evaluated. Previous literature has suggested that dispute management skills training is positively related to perceived self-efficacy and to higher expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior. Furthermore, perceptions of self-efficacy and expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior are positively related to social problem-solving competence. Finally, social problem-solving competence is positively related to nonviolent conflict resolution. The proposed model suggests only mediated relationships between dispute management training and nonviolent conflict resolution. Four hierarchical multiple regression equations were used to test the proposed model. These equations tested the following components: Equation 1: Dispute management training was evaluated for its predictive effect on perceived self-efficacy in interpersonal conflict situations. Equation 2: Dispute management training was evaluated for its predictive effect on the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior in interpersonal conflict situations. 66 Equation 3: Perceived self-efficacy and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior were evaluated for their predictive effects on social problem- solving competence (controlling for dispute management training). Equation 4: Social problem-solving competence was evaluated for its predictive effect on nonviolent conflict resolution in interpersonal conflict situations (controlling for dispute management training, perceived self-efficacy, and outcome expectations). In addition, when using hierarchical regression to test mediated effects, equations must be inverted and run backwards to double check the effects of variables or blocks of variables entered into the equation. To do this, Equation 4 was reversed: social problem-solving competence was entered first into the equation with nonviolent conflict resolution as the criteria; perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations were entered in the second block; and experimental condition was entered into the equation in block three. In hierarchical regression the relevant testis the significance of the change in the squared multiple R (R2 change), which indicates that the new variable (or block of variables) entered into the analysis adds significant predictive power, or variance, to the model above and beyond what other variables entered into the analysis already explain. For these analyses experimental condition was dummy coded along two dimensions: (1) whether students received training in dispute management skills, and (2) school setting. Therefore, the group that received dispute management training in an experimental school was coded (1,1). The group that received no training but was 67 in an experimental school was coded (0,1). Finally, the group that received no training and was in a control school was coded (0,0). As listed previously, Equation 1 tested the direct effect of dispute management training on perceptions of self-efficacy. Results indicated that dispute management training did not have a significant effect on perceived self-efficacy (E = .605, n = .55; R2 = .006, p = .55). Equation 2 evaluated the direct impact of dispute management training on the expectations of the outcome of competent behavior. Again, no significant effects were found (E = .116, p = .89; R2 = .002, p = .89). Tables 8 and 9, respectively, summarize these equations for the model and present the beta weights and their significance for variables in these equations. Equation 3 tested the direct effects of perceived self-efficacy and the expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior (the entrants) on social problem-solving competence (the criteria) with dispute management training as the control variable. This block of entrants had a significant direct effect on social problem-solving competence as indicated by a significant change in squared multiple R (E = 18.65, p < .0001; R2 = .365; R2 change = .305, p < .0001). Simultaneous testing of the two variables as indicated by the beta weights (Table 10) showed that perceived self- efficacy was a significant predictor of social problem-solving competence whereas expectations of the outcomes of competent behavior was not. These results indicated that perceived self-efficacy accounted for a significant portion of the variance associated with this block of entrants. In addition, there is a direct effect between dispute management training and social problem-solving competence for students who 68 Predictor Blocks 1’ B" t of t Block 1: Experimental Intervention F = .605 , p = .547 R’ = .006 R2 change = .006, p = .547 Trained condition .06 .063 .858 .392 Untrained condition -.07 .067 .921 .358 ' Correlations are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the measure. " Standardized regression coefficients are reported for the step in which they were entered into the model. Table 9 o‘r. ' .. u H‘ ("rs H a u. ' .H at .ur‘ «III int-.0 Ir- Q E . _ E . Z Olltmmncctations— Significance Predictor Blocks 1‘ [3" t of t Block 1: Experimental Intervention F = .ll6,p = .890 R’ = .002 R’change = .002, p = .890 Trained condition .04 .038 .420 .675 Untrained condition .03 .033 .365 .716 ‘ Correlations are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the measure. " Standardized regression coefficients are reported for the step in which they were entered into the model. 69 Table 10 Significance Predictor Blocks 1‘ B" t of t Block 1: Experimental Intervention F = 4.17, p < .05 R2 = .059 R2 change = .059, p < .05 Trained condition .30 .260 2. 89 .005 Untrained condition -. 15 .087 .97 .333 Block 2: Predictive Variables F = 18.65,p < .0001 R2 = .365 R2 change = .305,p < .0001 Outcome expectations .50 .093 1.27 .207 Perceptions of self-efficacy 1.09 .519 7.09 .000 ‘ Correlations are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the measure. " Standardized regression coefficients are reported for the step in which they were entered into the model. 70 receive training in nonviolent conflict resolution, but not for either control condition. Table 10 summarizes this equation for the proposed model and presents the beta weights and their significance for variables in the equation. Finally, Equation 4 evaluated the direct effects of social problem-solving competence on nonviolent conflict resolution. In Equation 4, nonviolent conflict resolution was tested as the criteria with social problem-solving competence as the entrant, and the other variables, perceived self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and dispute management training were control variables. If it is true that social problem- solving competence is the most proximal variable to nonviolent conflict resolution as depicted in the proposed model, when social problem-solving competence is entered into a hierarchical regression equation it should have significant predictive power on nonviolent conflict resolution as demonstrated by a significant change in the squared multiple R and a significant beta weight for the relationship between social problem- solving competence and nonviolent conflict resolution. Results indicated that social problem-solving competence had no impact on nonviolent conflict resolution above and beyond the variance accounted for by the control variables entered into the equation in the two previous blocks (E = 6.89, p < .0001; R2 = .212; R2 change = .0003, p = .81). However, results indicate that both perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations were significant predictors of nonviolent conflict resolution. Furthermore, dispute management training has a direct effect on nonviolent conflict resolution. Table 11 summarizes this equation of the 71 Table 11 u‘vlt’al I»: V II ‘ ("In ‘II : I. ° .II l.‘ .II‘ AM he; '1' \01 II M . . _ . W4 11 . 1 fl' 1 . Significance Predictor Blocks 7‘ B" t of t Block 1: Experimental Intervention F = 5.89,p < .05 R2 = .083 R2 change = .083, p < .05 Trained condition .14 .242 2.71 .007 Untrained condition .21 .260 2.92 .004 Block 2: Predictive Variables F = 8.67,p < .0001 R2 = .212 R’change = .129,p < .0001 Outcome expectations .44 .201 2.45 .016 Perceptions of self-efficacy .45 .247 3.01 .003 Block 3: Predictive Variables F = 6.89,p < .0001 R2 = .212 R’change = .0003,p = .814 Social competence .43 .023 .236 .814 ' Correlations are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the measure. " Standardized regression coefficients are reported for the step in which they were entered into the model. 72 proposed model represented in Figure 1 and presents the beta weights and their significance for variables in the equation. As a double check to the four equations described above, when using hierarchical regression to test a model the equations must be run backwards. To do so, Equation 4 was reversed, i.e. , social problem-solving competence was entered first into the equation with nonviolent conflict resolution as the criteria. If social problem-solving competence is the most proximal variable to nonviolent conflict resolution, and if it mediates the relationship between the other variables included in the model and nonviolent conflict resolution, results would indicate a significant direct effect between social problem-solving competence and nonviolent conflict resolution. Also, if social problem-solving competence is the most proximal variable, when other variables are entered into the equation after social problem-solving competence, i.e. , block two and three variables, there should be no significant changes in squared multiple R. In other words, if social problem-solving competence is the most proximal variable, it should explain enough variance that there is no variance lefi to be explained by variables entered in subsequent blocks. This analysis indicated that when entered first into the that the equation, social problem-solving competence had a significant effect on nonviolent conflict resolution (E = 8.97, p < .05; R2 = .064; p < .05) (see Table 12). However, when perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations were entered into the equation in block two, there was a significant change in squared multiple R, (E = 7.24, p < .05; R2 = .143; R2 change = .079, p < .05) and the relationship between social problem-solving 73 Table 12 Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis and Intercorrelations Predicting Nonviolent Conflict Resolution - Equation 4 Backwards ll . l C [1' B l . Significance Predictor Blocks 1‘ B” t of t Block 1: Predictive Variables F = 8.97 , p < .05 R2 = .063 R2 change = .063,p < .05 Social competence .43 .252 2.99 .003 Block 2: Predictive Variables F = 7.24, p < .05 R2 = .143 R2 change = .079, p < .05 Outcome expectations .44 .201 2.35 .020 Perceptions of self-efficacy .45 .206 2.07 .040 Block 3: Experiemental Intervention F = 6.89,p < .0001 R2 = .212 R2 change = .069, p = <.05 Trained condition . 14 .214 2.47 .015 Untrained condition .21 .252 3.00 .003 ‘ Correlations are corrected for attenuation due to unreliability in the measure. " Standardized regression coefficients are reported for the step in which they were entered into the model. 74 competence and nonviolent conflict resolution became nonsignificant. Perceived self- efficacy and outcome expectations accounted for variance above and beyond what social problem-solving competence and each demonstrated a significant direct effect on nonviolent conflict resolution. Finally, when experimental condition was entered into the equation there was a significant change in squared multiple R (E = 6.89, p < .0001; 32 = .212; 32 change = .069, p <.05). S EH' 1 . l B . ! l . The equations described above provide evidence that the mediated relationships in the hypothesized model are not supported. Instead, there are direct effects between perceived self-efficacy and nonviolent conflict resolution; between outcome expectations and nonviolent conflict resolution; and between experimental condition and nonviolent conflict resolution for students who are trained in dispute management skills and for students who attend an experimental school who are not trained in conflict resolution. Furthermore, there is a direct relationship between dispute management training and social problem-solving competence. Figure 2 shows a revised model of the direct effects between experimental school, perceived self- efficacy, outcome expectations, and nonviolent conflict resolution. Summary of Results An additional AN OVA conducted to investigate the effects of dispute management training on nonviolent conflict resolution was significant (E (2, 206) = 7.24, p < .05; n2 = .066). Scheffé post hoc multiple comparison procedure indicated that students trained in dispute management skills (M = 3.48, SD = .67) and students who attended 75 a treatment school who did not receive training (M = 3.49, SD = .65) were significantly more nonviolent than students who attended a control school (M = 3.07 , SD = .93). There were no significant differences between groups of students who attended the treatment schools with respect to nonviolent conflict resolution. The findings from analyses of variance and the results of hierarchical regression are consistent. Instead of the mediated relationships as hypothesized in the proposed model, results indicate that there are direct effects between variables as shown in Figure 2. 76 neuaemom 85.50 332282 J K 382 3:39: ”N 2:5 83:38me 08038 % 58533 f 838qu Begum 535235 Chapter Four DISCUSSION Results of this study do not appear to support the efficacy of the dispute management training program which was evaluated. Although receiving training in conflict resolution skills was significantly related to higher social problem-solving competence, this relationship was not related to nonviolent conflict resolution. Furthermore, attending a school with a dispute management training program was significantly related to nonviolent conflict resolution. However, it is precarious to attribute these findings solely to the presence of the training program in the school. It is possible that both trained and untrained students in the experimental schools displayed less violent conflict resolution techniques because students who were not trained learned nonviolent conflict resolution techniques from their peers who received training. In other words, the dispute management program could have had radiating effects whereby students who received training positively influenced the school environment and their friends who did not directly participate in training. It appears more likely, however, that this finding can be explained by the fact that there were differences between the experimental and control schools prior to the implementation of the dispute management program. The chi-squared test for independence presented earlier indicated that there were no significant differences between the number of students who were violent or who committed illegal acts in the treatment and control schools. However, in contrast to this analysis, an analysis of variance run on survey data collected during wave one of 77 78 the overall evaluation indicated that there were differences between the schools prior to implementation of the dispute management intervention (for content of questions see Table 13). This AN OVA revealed that students at the control schools reported participating in more delinquent or antisocial behavior in their schools than did students at the treatment schools (M = 1.2, SD = .79; M = 1.1, SD = .75 respectively); (E (1,1457) = 8.9, p< .05; I12 = .006). Based upon these findings, it is difficult to attribute the significant difference between treatment and control schools as effects of the dispute management training program. Although the findings of this study do not support the utility of the intervention evaluated in this investigation they do support previous research on the importance of efficacy and outcome expectations and shed light on the processes involved in promoting nonviolent conflict resolution. Consistent with previous research on other high-risk behaviors, this study confirms the theoretical importance of perceived self- efficacy and outcome expectations when trying to understand how adolescents act and perceive themselves in interpersonal interactions. Results indicated that both perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations were significantly related to nonviolent conflict resolution. These findings confirm previous research which suggests that both self- efficacy and outcome expectations are important indicators of behavior (Bandura, 1982; Connolly, 1989; Wheeler & Ladd, 1982). Self—efficacy has been related to a variety of social abilities including making friends, developing intimate bonds, being socially assertive, participating in social groups (Connolly, 1989; Ford, 1982) and now to nonviolent conflict resolution. Self- 79 Table 13 IIII I .1 '.‘III I "le a‘ .‘v.I I I" A- ‘CJI' I I‘ hm: Question Since the beginning of this January, how many times have you: Got into a fist fight Talked your way out of a fight Hit someone who made fun of you Messed up school property Been sent to the principal's office for bad behavior Carried a gun or knife to school Skipped a class Skipped school for a whole day Stopped people from fighting each other 10. Went out of your way to help someone 11. Threatened to hurt someone 12. Been suspended or excluded from school 13. Helped a teacher during class 14. Had someone physically assault and hurt you at school 15. Had someone take something from you using physical force 16. Had something stolen from your locker 17. Had someone threaten to hurt you at school a = . 8406 PPHP‘E’W‘P’PI‘ 80 efficacy has also been linked to social problem-solving competence (Allen, et al. 1990; Caplan et al. 1992), a relationship which is supported by these current findings. This study indicates that an individual’s evaluations of his or her capability to act nonviolently are important to his or her ability to effectively resolve an immediate conflict in a manner that also reduces the chance that the individual will be faced with a similar situation in the future (social competence). Individuals who perceive themselves as efficacious are more likely to resolve conflicts nonviolently. Previous research (e.g. , Allen et al. 1990), furthermore, indicates that an individual’s belief in his or her ability to perform a given behavior (self-efficacy) lacks meaning unless the individual also believes that performing the behavior will lead to a desired outcome (outcome expectations). Consistent with this research, outcome expectations in this study had a significant direct effect on nonviolent conflict resolution. These findings indicate that adolescents who positively evaluated the outcome of resolving interpersonal conflicts nonviolently were more apt to behave nonviolently. Given that perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations demonstrated significant direct effects on nonviolent conflict resolution it is important to consider these constructs when planning programs to reduce violent behavior. When seeking to understand the apparent lack of efficacy of this intervention it is important to consider factors related to program conceptualization or implementation failure. Program failure can occur when the theory behind a program does not fit with the processes underlying targeted behavior change. Although the current study did not provide support for the hypothesized model there is evidence which indicates that the 81 underlying relationships described in the model may still explain the processes whereby dispute management training can influence nonviolent behavior. It is likely, therefore, that faulty theoretical explanations are not a problem for this current study or for the conflict resolution training program. However, multiple issues regarding the implementation of the training program should be considered. Implementation failure occurs when a program is not implemented effectively. Such failure can occur when a program modeled after a pilot program is not replicated the way the original program intended. Implementation failure and threats to program integrity are many and include: the relevance of information being presented to the target population; attributes of the target population including demographic characteristics such as age, sex, and grade in school, and environmental aspects such as the socio-economic conditions of the area surrounding the school; level of support from program sponsors; capabilities of the program facilitators; and consistency with which the curriculum is delivered to students. One of the major concerns regarding the implementation of this program centers around the lack of consistency of program delivery. On multiple occasions throughout the ten-week training session the facilitators were absent on training days. Interruptions in training can lead to decreased effectiveness as students forget information from one training session to the next. In addition, lack of consistency and reliability on the part of the facilitator may give students the impression that the training is not important. One indication of implementation failure in this study is the difference between the number of students who indicated by self-report that they had received training in conflict resolution skills 82 (38 students) and the number of students who provided a homeroom number which corresponded to a homeroom receiving training (67 students), see Table 1. This discrepancy suggests that students who received training were unaware of the program, an indication that the program may not have made a significant impression upon the students. Because of such implementation issues it is difficult to draw conclusions about the potential effectiveness of the dispute management training. Current findings demonstrate that this dispute management training program did not enhance adolescents’ self-efficacy and outcome expectations. It may be, however, that more effectively implemented conflict resolution programs are able to positively affect adolescents’ perceived self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and social problem- solving competence, and by doing so reduce their violent conflict. Summary This study provides information on variables that are important to understanding nonviolent conflict resolution. The results of this study indieate the underlying theoretical importance of self-efficacy and outcome expectations in nonviolent conflict resolution. These findings are consistent with the literature on social learning theory and research on reducing high-risk behaviors and provide an important contribution by bridging previous knowledge to the understanding of nonviolent conflict resolution. A number of caveats should be considered in relation to this investigation. Issues related to measurement may have been problematic. It is important to note that the significant relationship between perceived self-efficacy and social problem-solving competence may be difficult to interpret because of the high intercorrelation and 83 multicolinearity of these two variables (when corrected for attenuation r = 1.09). Although the interview protocol and scenarios utilized in this study are based upon previous instruments used by Allen et al. (1990) future research could benefit by further developing these constructs to limit high intercorrelations and by increasing scale reliabilities of constructs used in the model. Also, because the data used in this evaluation are all based upon self-report measures they are subject to possible participant biases. The validity of self-report data can be threatened by a respondent’s desire to report socially acceptable, rather than actual, information (Crano & Brewer, 1986). Future evaluation efforts may avoid such biases by including other sources of data, for example, the number of conflicts reported to teachers and administrators as well as the number of conflicts reported by students on self-report measures. Other potential limitations of this study include low power (i.e. the probability of correctly rejecting the null hypothesis) which may have compromised the ability to recognize effects that would have been significant if power were higher. Small sample size relative to the effect sizes of the independent variables on the dependent variables contributed to decreased power. Future studies should include a larger sample. In addition, the use of a post-only design warrants discussion. The posttest—only design was chosen because of the expense involved in tracking students in a pretest-posttest design. Unfortunately, data collected from surveys administered during the pre-test phase of the overall evaluation and analyses of variance indicate that the schools may have been different prior to the intervention. These differences confound conclusions drawn from this study. 34 Future Directions The prevalence of violence in our society and its impact on schools and students is undeniable. To date there is a lack of empirical evidence that school conflict resolution programs effectively reduce violent resolutions to interpersonal conflict (Tolan & Guerra, 1994a; Webster, 1993). However, there is anecdotal evidence that social problem-solving programs can promote gains in targeted cognitive skills and beliefs about nonviolent conflict resolution (Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). Because personnel and monetary resources continue to be devoted to conflict resolution programs in schools (Webster, 1993), and because of the far-reaching deleterious effects of violence in schools, further research which includes strong outcome measures and which documents the process and ongoing implementation of training programs is vital. This exploratory investigation attempted to explain the process of reducing conflict by applying new constructs to the evaluation of a dispute management program. These constructs, social problem-solving competence, perceived self- efficacy, and outcome expectations, have been found to be important in understanding change with respect to other antisocial behavior (e. g. substance abuse, unprotected sexual intercourse). This current investigation provides support for the theoretical importance of perceived self-efficacy and outcome expectations in understanding nonviolent conflict resolution. Further research and evaluation on nonviolent conflict resolution should consider these variables when attempting to reduce violent conflict. Although the dispute management training program did not impact either of these self- expectations, the lack of influence may be due to failures in program implementation. 85 Other programs may have more impact on fostering perceptions of self-efficacy and the outcome of competent behavior and by doing so may be able to directly increase the use of nonviolent techniques to resolve interpersonal conflict. One of the more promising developments in the field of conflict resolution is the implementation of community-based outreach and edueation programs which aim to combat violence in all areas of an adolescent's life (Commission on Violence & Youth, 1993; DeJong, 1994; Elliott, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). These programs are comprehensive in nature and attempt to bring together all primary institutions that serve youth, including: families, health agencies, schools, employers, and juvenile justice systems (Elliott, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). The inclusion of all relevant institutions is a common-sense approach to reducing violence in our communities and may be more effective in reducing violence programs which target only one institution, such as school-based programs. As with school conflict resolution programs, however, there is no empirical evidence to support the effectiveness of these programs (Elliott, 1994; Tolan & Guerra, 1994a). It is clear that more research and evaluation is needed to effectively reduce violence in our communities. This current study may provide insight to variables critical to understanding nonviolent conflict resolution. However, only through stringent evaluations that include clear behavioral outcome variables and thorough documentation of program implementation and delivery will the true effectiveness of programs be realized. APPENDICES APPENDIX A Appendix A Coding Manual for Scenarios -- Social Problem-Solving Competence 115" [5.12“ -51' D Social problem-solving competence is often defined in relation to success in resolving interpersonal conflicts. In these scenarios, a maximally competent response is one that effectively resolves the immediate social conflict and reduces the chances that the subject will have to face similar problems of this type in the future. Conversely, an incompetent response is one that does not effectively resolve the immediate social conflict and may be likely to cause more problems for the subject in the future. mm 8 -- a very competent or very effective response 6 -- a competent or effective response 4 -- a response that is neither competent nor incompetent, a response that neither directly helps nor hurts the situation or individual 2 -- an incompetent or ineffective response 0 -- a very incompetent or ineffective response NOTE: Various coding levels require that one or more conditions be met in a subject's response in order to be coded at that specific level. These areas are indicated with AND between the different qualifications. Others will indicate OR. In these instances different responses qualify for the coding level. W: Unless otherwise directed a response of ”Don't Know” should be coded as very incompetent or very ineffective and is therefore coded as a zero. "Don't Know" is different from a response in which a subject says he or she would do nothing. In many instances doing nothing is an understandable approach. Mmethanmmsponse; If the subject says that he/ she would do tWo or more responses which would normally receive different scores and does not choose one over the other as a final answer, the subject should receive the score of the lowest response. If the subject gives one or more responses in a series or sequence of responses (e. g. ”I would first try to talk it out and if that did not work I would try something else”) the response should be coded as a totality of its parts according to the elements given. PhysicaLharm; Any response in which the subject says he/ she would use physical force or fight should receive a score of zero. 86 87 Scandal Your mother has been nagging you about getting home immediately after school. One day you get home an hour late and your mother yells at you, "Do you know what time it is? Where have you been?” NOTE: For 4 - 8, student may state a specific place or activity he! she was doing and therefore was late. If the response seems that this is a normal and acceptable activity then it should not be taken as an excuse or a lie. For example, "I stayed at school to help a teacher or I was at a friends house. So she wouldn't worry.” Instead of saying " I would tell her where I was, " this first student gave examples of why he/ she could be late. In this case these reasons are not excuses or lies but examples. 8 - Student explains why he or she is late. AND Student is empathetic towards the mother and shows concern for her wishes or feelings. Student may apologize. Students may state his/ her concern with telling the truth as the reason for explaining. The main or only reason the student is explaining should not be solely to avoid getting into further trouble. Examples: I'd tell my mother where I was and what I was doing because I know she was worried. I‘d say I got caught in the wrong place and was not able to call home right away. She won't think I was getting hurt or didn't care about coming home, I didn't want her to worry. I'd tell her where I was because it is the truth. 4 - Student gives an explanation but does not show concern for mother's feelings or wishes. Student simply answers the mother' s question with no mention of his/ her tardiness being a problem. OR Student states desire to not get into further trouble. OR Student states that he! she would answer the mother just to answer her question. OR Student apologizes but offers no explanation of where he/ she has been. Examples: Tell her where I've been because she asked. Tell her where I was so I won't get into more trouble. Tell the truth so I won't get into trouble. 3 - Student gives an explanation but shows no concern for mother' s feelings and wishes. Student may sound rude and state the reason for explaining is to have the mother stop asking. Myles: Tell her where I was so she would stop hollering. Tell her I was with my friends so she would quit yelling at me. Explain the situation to get her off my back. V 88 0 - Student does not give an explanation for tardiness. Student ignores mother. Student says he/ she would not do anything. Student states that it “would depend. " Student lies or makes up excuses to avoid getting in trouble. Student says "Don't Know” Examples: I don't say anything. I just go to my room. "I've been out. " Nothing. I'd take my punishment. Make up a story because I was probably somewhere my mother wouldn't want me to be. I lost track of time because I don't have a watch. Say that the bus was late so I wouldn't get in bigger trouble. Seem A kid you know is a drug dealer. He found out that school security is going to check his locker after the next class. He asks you to keep his stuff in your locker. 8 - Student assertively refuses to keep the drugs in his or her locker. Student refuses and explains why not as being a refusal to be involved with drugs or people who are involved with drugs. Student may say that he or she would report it to an authority (teacher, principal, security officer) NOTE: If a student says that he/ she would tell an authority and wishes to avoid trouble score as a 8. Student says N O and that he or she would change friends. Examples: 1' d say noandwalkawaybecauseldon' tlikedrugs. I' (1 go tell the principal because it is bad to have it. My friends wouldn' t have it or we would quit being friends. I' (1 tell him no and tell him to turn the drugs 111. I wouldn t want something to happen to him. I'd tell him no and tell the security officer what he asked me to do. I wouldn't want to get in trouble or him to get in trouble. 4 - Student refuses to keep the drugs in his or her locker and expresses fear of being eaught by an authority figure as the reason for not keeping the drugs. Student tells the dealer to find someone else or to find another place to store the drugs. 89 Examples: I'd say no because I don't want to get caught with it. I'd say no because it is stupid, I'd be getting in trouble for someone else. No, he has to keep his own stuff, they would probably check my locker and I don't want to get in trouble. I'd probably say to give it to someone else. I wouldn't want to get caught. I'd say no because he might give someone else my locker combination. 3 - Student says yes but expresses intentions of turning the drugs in, not of really storing them. Examples: I'd let him keep it in there and then tell the counselor. So I wouldn't get in trouble and he wouldn't bring it anymore. 0 - Student says that he or she would keep the drugs in his/her locker. Student says that he or she will hide the drugs elsewhere for the dealer. Student agrees to keep the drugs but only if he or she can have some of it or some of the profits of sale for his or her help. Student says ”Don't Know” Examples: I'd say yes, I wouldn't want him getting busted. If it's a cousin I'd say yes. If it's just a friend I'd say no. If it's a cousin I wouldn't want him to get into trouble. If it's just a friend, it's his responsibility. I'd say yes but only if he would give me some. Samaria}: You think that one of your classmates has recently been telling rumors about you. You don't know this student well, so you don't know why he (she) is telling these rumors. You're upset about the rumors and you want the student to stop telling them. 8 - Student says that he/ she would like to talk over the situation with the other student. Student says that he! she would ask the other student why he/ she was telling the rumors. Student says that he! she would not pay attention to the rumors because rumors are not worth it. Ignores the situation. Student states that he! she would tell an authority only after one of the above means of resolving the situation is attempted and fails, e. g. only if necessary. Examples: I'd ask him why he keeps telling these rumors on me and if he didn't stop I'd tell the teacher. I don't see why he's spreading rumors about me because most of them aren't true. I’d go to the person and ask them about it and talk it over because if she's spreading rumors I'd want them to stop. I really don't care about rumors because they are not true. What's the use. If they're not true why worry about them. 90 6 - Student would ASK the other student to stop but does not say he/ she would talk about the situation, only that he/ she would ask the other student to cease telling rumors. Student states that he/ she would tell an authority only after one of the above means of resolving the situation is attempted and fails, e. g. only if necessary. Examples: I'd ask him to quit telling rumors, it's not polite. I'd ask her politely to stop because I don't want to get in a fight. I'd ask him to stop and if not, ask a teacher to help. 4 - Student states that he/ she would TELL the other student to stop telling rumors. Student states that he/ she would approach the other person in an aggressive manner as if giving an order to stop telling rumors. Student states that he! she would go to an authority or another person first (without trying to talk to the other student). Student describes a roundabout way of dealing with the situation. Examples: I'd confront her so she'd stop. I'd say leave me alone and stop spreading rumors about me. I'd tell the teacher or principal because it's wrong to spread rumors. I'd go talk to someone and ask them to find the student and see what they can do then I would talk to the student and ask them why because I don't want nobody telling rumors about me. 2 - Student says that he! she would threaten the other student. Student states that he/ she would talk to the other student but then states that he! she would hit the other student. Examples: I'd ask him to stop to scare him. I'd go up to him and let him know not to mess with me. I'd ask him if he said it to make sure it's him and not someone else so I won't start a fight with the wrong person. I'd confront him and ask him why and if he doesn't answer it's a fist fight. 0 - Student states that he/ she would fight the other student. Student responds with "Don't know.” Examples: I'd hit him in the mouth because he talks too much. 91 W You are walking home alone down a dark street. Two boys (girls) are following you. You don't know them but they are about your age. They catch up with you and say, "Give us your jacket. " 8 - Student says that he/ she would give them the jacket. Examples: I'd give them the jacket because I don't want to get hurt. A jacket is not worth risking your life over. I'd give them the jacket. Why should I fight for it? 6 - Student would give them the jacket only after trying to determine if they had a weapon or only after he! she sees a weapon. Examples: If they had a weapon I'd give it to them. I'd say something and if they pulled out a weapon I'd give them the jacket. 4 - Student says that he/she would walk away, ignore them, say no, run away, ask them why (with no further action), etc.. Examples: I'd say no and then I'd keep on walking because I don't want to get hurt. I'd ignore them and run away. I won't give it, they won't kill me. I'd say no, I need my jacket. 0 - Student states that he/ she would fight the other teenagers. Student responds with ”Don't know.” Examples: I'd fight and then run. To protect myself. I'd hit them. Scenario}: You are sitting with your friends in the lunch room. A boy (girl) you don't know spills his (her) drink on you. One of your friends tells him (her) to apologize and he (she) refuses. 8 - Student says that he/ she would walk away, let it go, ignore it, not worry about it. Student says that he! she would get something to wipe it up. Student tells his/her friend not to worry about it. 92 Examples: I'd tell him he doesn't have to apologize. I'll just clean it up because I don't want to go through no trouble. I'd forget about it because if it was a mistake I wouldn't care if it was on purpose I'd be mad. I wouldn't say anything but I'd be mad, it always happens in food fights. 6 - Student states that he! she would ask the other student to apologize or to help clean it up. Student would try to determine if it was an accident or on purpose and if on purpose would tell an authority. Student would ask why but does not state any further action. Student says he! she would ask for an apology but if the other student refused it would be okay. Examples: Can you please apologize or get something to get this off of me. If a person spills on you they're supposed to say sorry. Ask her to get it off of me because she spilled it. I'd ask her why she did it and go tell someone. I'd ask her why. I'd ask him to apologize but if he didn't I won't make a big deal about it because I wouldn't want to get in a fight. 4 - Student states that he/ she would TELL the other student to apologize. Student states that he/ she would TELL the other student to get something to wipe it up. Student states that he/ she would tell an authority first before any other attempt at resolution. Examples: I'd tell him to get me something to wipe it up with. I'd tell her to say sorry. I'd tell on him because he spilled his juice on me. 3 - Student states that he/ she would threaten the other student. Student states that he! she would yell at the other student. Student states that he! she would TELL the other student to buy replacement clothing. Examples: I'd get angry and yell. I'd tell her that she better wash my shirt and apologize. I'd be mad at her. 2 - Student would determine it was on purpose or an accident and only if on purpose would he/ she fight. Student states that he! she would ask for an apology or help cleaning and if none, only then would fight. Examples: I'd tell him to get it off me and if he didn't I'd fight. 93 If it was an accident, okay. If on purpose, I'd hit him. 0 - Student states that he/ she would fight or throw a drink back at the other student. Student responds with ”Don't know." Examples: I'd fight because he didn't apologize. I'd throw it back at him. Seemm You are at school and you see another student talking to your boyfriend (girlfriend). They are laughing and smiling and seem to be having fun. Suddenly, your boyfriend (girlfriend) looks at you and turns quickly away. 8 - Student states that he! she would wait until he/ she is in private with their girlfriend/boyfriend to talk about the situation. Student acknowledges that it is okay to be friends with other opposite sex people without it being a problem. Student states that he/ she would join the conversation BUT is not doing so because he/she is mad, instead is just interested in joining the two. Examples: I wouldn't do anything, it's okay to be friends. I'd wait until he's alone and ask who she was. Ask what they were talking about later in private to avoid a fight. I'd walk over there to see what they said. I'd ask her what they are talking about. I'd ask him what was that all about. It's better to ask, I might be thinking the wrong thing. 4 - Student states that he/ she would ask the boyfiiend/ girlfriend who the other person is and what they are talking about AND expresses suspicion about the situation. Student states that he/ she would be jealous or mad. Student states that he/ she wouldn't do anything but is suspicious and assumes the worst of the situation. Student states that he/ she would not fight over girls/boys. Examples: I'd ask him what he was doing; if he was just talking I'd forget about it. If more, I'd dump him. After they get through I'd ask her why she turned away from me. The other boy might be trying to turn her away from me. I wouldn't do anything but I'd be mad. I'd keep on walking. He's ignoring me and talking to her so I'll do the same. I'd ask him why he's talking to her, he's not supposed to talk to other girls. 2 - Student states that he/ she would dump the boyfriend/ girlfriend. 94 Student does not consider the idea that they may just be friends and makes no mention of gathering more information. Student mentions being wronged by the boyfriend/ girlfriend. Student states that he/ she would yell at the boyfriend/ girlfriend. Student states that he/ she would try to make boyfriend/ girlfriend jealous. W: I wouldn't do anything, I'm finished with him. I'd tell her I don't like her anymore because she is talking to other boys. 0 - Student states that he/ she would fight. Student responds with "Don't know.” Examples: I'd fight who she is talking to. APPENDIX B Appendix B Coding Manual for Scenarios - Outcome Expectations Outcome expectations as defined by Bandura (1977), refer to an individual's estimate that a given performed behavior will lead to a desired outcome. Outcome expectations are related to, but separate from, social competence and perceptions of self-efficacy. WSW: 5 -- expectation of a very positive outcome 4 -- expectation of a somewhat positive outcome 3 -- expectation of a neutral outcome 2 -- expectation of a somewhat negative outcome 1 -- expectation of a very negative outcome Directions: For each scenario, use the following coding schemes to rate responses. For a response to be coded at a level it must be similar to any of the responses described by that level. Note that the difference between levels of responses often includes specific references to how the student describes the outcome in relation to him/herself. For example, in Scenario 2, level 3 includes, ”The dealer would be mad, " whereas level 4 includes, ”The dealer would be mad but I'd be okay. " The difference in level is reflective of the student' s attention to him/herself. In the same vein, level 2 includes, ”Dealer would be mad and not want to be friends.” Again, this response differs from level 3; in level 2 the student's specific reference to him/ herself is more negative than in level 3. Morethanpneresppnse: If the subject gives two or more potential outcomes which would normally receive different scores and does not choose one over the other as a final answer, the final rating should be the score of the lowest response. If the subject gives one or more outcomes in a series or sequence the response should be coded as a totality of its parts according to the elements given. Note: For the purposes of coding, responses containing words such as ”probably,” ”maybe,” "might," etc. will be coded the same as similar responses without such words. Ehxsicallrarm; Any response in which the subject states the outcome includes physical harm to him/herself should be scored as a one. 95 96 Seenafiml Your mother has been nagging you about getting home immediately after school. One day you get home an hour late and your mother yells at you, ”Do you know what time it is? Where have you been?" CODING CRITERIA 5 - Student expects mother to understand; to not punish; to forgive; to believe. Student does not expect to get in any trouble. Student says nothing will happen. Example; She'd say okay and I wouldn't get in any trouble. 4 - Student expects mother to warn him/her about future occurrences. Student does not expect to get in any trouble. Example; She'd tell me to get home earlier next time. 3 -- Student does not expect to get in trouble. Mother may/ may not be mad. Mother will stop yelling. Example; She may or may not be mad. 2 -- Mother may yell but student does not expect to get in too much trouble. Mother may check out the truth of the story. Mother may not listen/believe student. Example; She'd yell but I probably wouldn't get into too much trouble. 1 -- Student expects to get in trouble Example; I'd be grounded. Seem A kid you know is a drug dealer. He found out that school security is going to check his locker after the next class. He asks you to keep his stuff in your locker. CODING CRITERIA 5 -- Student expects dealer to leave him/ her alone; to find someone else. Student states that the dealer may get in trouble but student is okay. Student will not get in trouble. Nothing dealer could do. Student states that nothing would happen. Example; He'd leave me alone. 97 4 - Student expects dealer would get mad but student would be okay. Student says dealer may ”get an attitude" but leave student alone and find someone else to keep the drugs. Student states dealer would get in trouble. Example; He'd get mad at me but I'd be okay. 3 -- Student says dealer would be mad. Example; He'd be mad. 2 - Student says dealer would get mad and not want to be friends. Dealer would continue to try to persuade student. Dealer would bully student. Example; He'd keep pestering me to keep his stuff. 1 -- Student expects dealer would fight or threaten to fight. Dealer would tell an authority that the drugs belonged to the student. Dealer would force student to keep the drugs. Example; He'd probably hit me and make me do it. Samaria} You think that one of your classmates has recently been telling rumors about you. You don't know this student well, so you don't know why he (she) is telling these rumors. You're upset about the rumors and you want the student to stop telling them. CODING CRITERIA 5 -- Student expects the rumor-teller to apologize; to stop telling rumors. The student states that the situation is rectified by the rumor-teller denouncing the truth of the rumors. Example; She'd apologize and stop telling the rumors. 4 -- Student expects to find out why the rumor-teller was spreading rumors; expects confession from the rumor-teller; gets an explanation. Example; She'd tell me why. 98 3 -- Student does not expect situation to change; does not expect the rumor-teller to listen. Student states the outcome would depend upon the person telling rumors. A fight would be avoided; the rumor-teller may or may not explain why he/ she is telling rumors. Student states that nothing would happen. Example; The rumors probably won't stop. 2 - Student expects that the rumor-teller would have a "bad attitude” and may start cussing or arguing; the rumor-teller would lie or deny the rumor. Student states that the situation might get worse. Student states that he! she would be humiliated or embarrassed. Example; She'd start yelling and cussing. 1 -- Student expects rumor-teller will try to fight or will want to fight. Example; He'd probably hit me. SeenapipA You are walking home alone down a dark street. Two boys (girls) are following you. You don't know them but they are about your age. They catch up with you and say, ”Give us your jacket. " CODING CRITERIA 5 -- Student expects to keep the jacket; to be left alone by the two others; to be able to leave the situation; to be safe; able to get home. Examples; They'd leave me alone. I'd be safe even if I don't have my jacket. 4 -- Student expects to be in a better position after giving them his/her jacket. Example; I'd be better off. 3 -- Student says they would take the jacket and leave. Student states he/ she could get another jacket. Student states that nothing would happen. Example; They'd leave with the jacket. 2 -- Student is not physically hurt but the situation is not over. Student states that he/ she is humiliated; that they laugh at him/her; that they bully him/her; that they follow him/ her. Student states that he/ she would be cold. Examples; They'd make fun of me. 99 I'd be cold. 1 - Student expects that they would try to fight; beat him/her up; want something else; push; knock student down. Student states that he/ she would get in trouble at home. Examples; They would ask me for my shoes. I'd get in trouble with my mom. Seenarlpj You are sitting with your friends in the lunch room. A boy (girl) you don't know spills his (her) drink on you. One of your friends tells him (her) to apologize and he (she) refuses. CODING CRITERIA 5 -- Student expects the drink-spiller to apologize; to offer to clean up the mess. Student is satisfied with the outcome; expresses resolution of the situation. Example; She'd apologize and help clean up. 4 -- Student does not expect an apology but states that it does not matter. Student states that a fight would be avoided. Student states that he/ she would be left alone. Example; He'd go sit down and not apologize but I don't care. 3 - Student and spiller go separate ways; say nothing; student states that he/ she would wipe up the mess him/herself. Student states that it depend upon the person who spilled the drink; says he! she would forget it. Student states that nothing would happen. Example; He'd go sit down and I'd wipe up the mess. 2 -- Student states that he/ she would be humiliated; that the spiller or the student' 3 friends would make fun of him/her; that he/ she would be picked on by spiller or friends. Student states that spiller does not apologize. Example; My friends would call me weak. 1 -- Student expects a fight; expects that spiller or his/her friends will spill on him/her again. Example; He might do it again. 100 W You are at school and you see another student talking to your boyfriend (girlfriend). They are laughing and smiling and seem to be having fun. Suddenly, your boyfriend (girlfriend) looks at you and turns quickly away. CODING CRITERIA 5 -- Student expects to work out the situation with his/her girl/boyfriend. Talk things out. Expects girl/boyfriend to stop talking to the other student. Student expects to become friends with the third student. Example; She'd wait after school so we could talk about everything. 4 -- Student expects to find out who the other student is. Example; My boyfriend would tell me who she was. 3 -- Student states nothing would happen. Says a fight would be avoided. Situation is not better and not worse. Example; Nothing would get started in school. 2 -- Student says that girl/boyfriend would get angry; girl/boyfriend would say ”leave me alone”; would walk away; not wait after school. Girl/boyfriend might " get an attitude” or yell. They might or might not talk. Example; She'd yell and be angry. I -- Student expects to break up; fight. Example; She'd dump me. REFERENCES REFERENCES Agnew, R. (1993). Why do they do it? An examination of the intervening mechanisms between ”social control” variables and delinquency. leumalpLReseareh erimeandJlelinquenstflO), 245-266 Akers,R- L. (1985). WWWGM ed.)- Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Allen, J. P., Leadbeater, B. J ., & Aber, J. L. (1990). The relationship of adolescents' expectations and values to delinquency, hard drug use, and unprotected sexual intercourse. W 85-98. Allen, J. P., Weissberg, R. P., & Hawkins, J. A. (1989). The relation between values and social competence in early adolescence. We), 45 8-464. Araki, C. T. (1990). Dispute management in the schools. Mediationflranmlx. 8(1), 51-62. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. W0), 191-215- Bandura, A. (1982). Self—efficacy mechanism in human agency. American WC). 122-147. Bandura, A. ,Adams, N. E. H,ardy, A. R. ,&Howells, G. N. (1980). Tests of the generality of self-efficacy theory. W 39—66. Bastian, L. D., &Taylor, B. M. (1991). Schoolsrime:_A_national_caime WW (Report No. NCJ 131645). Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Justice. Bronfenbrenner, U- (1979). W hanamreandnesign. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cameron, J., & Dupuis, A. (1991). The introduction of school mediation to New Zealand. W240). 1 13. 101 102 Caplan, M. ,Weissberg, R. P., Grober, J. 8., Sivo, P. J, Grady, K., & Jacoby, C. (1992). Social competence with inner-city and suburban young adolescents: Effects on social adjustment and alcohol use. .lnumamffionsulnnLandEhnis‘al Esxehologm 56-63. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1990). Homicide among young black males- United States, 1978 1987. MorbidinLandMonalituMeeklrLRemn. 39(48), 869-873. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1992). Physical fighting among high school students- United States, 1990. IoumalpnheAmericanMcdit‘al W02), 3009. Christie, D. J ., & Toomey, B. G. (1990). The stress of violence: School, community, and world. In L. E. Arnold (Ed.), Childhrxxljtress (pp. 297-323). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Clark, P. M. (1994). Preventing violence by building competent communities. CommunithewsflienLlQ). 3-4 Commission on Violence and Youth. (1993). W W Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Connolly, J. (1989). Social self-efficacy rn adolescence: Relations with self- concept, social adjustment, and mental health. Canadianleumalpflfiehayieuml We), 258-269. Crano, W. D.. & Brewer. M. B. (1986). ErinciplesandMethcdmLsttcial Research (pp. 40-57). Newton, MA: Allyn and Bacon, Inc. Crary, D. R. (1992). Community benefits from mediation: A test of the ”Peace Virus” hypothesis. W220), 24 -252. DeJong,W. (1994). WWW . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Eisenberg, D. S., & Harris, J. D. (1984). Social competence: A developmental PCISPOCtive. W30). 241-252- Elliott, D. S. (1994). MW Boulder, CO: Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence. 103 Farrington, D. P. (1991). Childhood aggression and adult violence: Early precursors and later-life outcomes. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.). The development and treatment of childhool aggression (pp. 5-29). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fingerhut, L. A, Ingram, D. D., & Feldman, J. J. (1992). Firearm homicide among black teenage males 1n metropolitan counties. IoumalpnheAmerieanMedical W02), 3054-3058. Ford, M. E. (1982). Social cognition and social competence in adolescence. Dexelopmentalflrtehalogrtalfic), 323-340. Freedman, B. J. Rosenthal, L, Donohoe, C. P, Schlundt, D. G., & McFall, R. M. (197 8) A social-behavioral analysis of skill deficits in delinquent and nondelinquent boys. WWWAS. 1448-1462 Gaffney, L. R., & McFall, R. M. (1981). A comparison of social skills in delinquency and nondelinquency adolescent girls using a behavioral role-playing inventory. Wfi), 959-967. Grusec, J. E. (1992). Social learning theory and developmental psychology: The legacies of Robert Sears and Albert Bandura. WW (5). 776 - 786. Hausman, A. J., Spivack, H., Prothrow-Stith, D., & Roeber, J. (1992). Patterns of teen exposure to a community-based violence prevention program. Jpnmalpf AdolescentflealthJZ. 668-675. Hellman, D. A, & Beaton, S. (1986). The pattern of violence in urban public schools: The influence of school and community. lpumalpresearehjnfirimeand W0). 102- 127 Hirschi, T. (1971). W. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Hrarritz, J. R. ,& Eddowes, E. A. (1990). Violence: A crisis in homes and schools. ChridhnmlJiducatronJl. 4-7. Johnson, D. W, & Johnson, R. T. (1995). Why violence prevention programs don’ t work -- and what does. we), 63-67. Johnson, D. W. ,,Johnson R. T., & Dudley, B. (1992). Effects of peer mediation training on elementary school students. Medratmnflnarterlrtamu), 89- 99. 104 Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T. Dudley, B. ,& Acikgoz, K. (1994). Effects of conflict resolution training on elementary school students. IheleumalpLSpeial Wm), 803- 817. Johnson, D. W. ,Johnson, R. T., &Stevahn, L. (1995, July). W WWW Paper presented at the annual meetings of the National Association for Mediation 1n Education, Seattle, WA. Kazdin, A. 13., Bass, D., Siegel, T, & Thomas, C. (1989). Cognitive-behavioral therapy and relationship therapy 1n the treatment of children referred for antisocial behavior. laumalnLConsulnnundflmrcaIPsxchologLflm). 522-535. Luthar, S. S. (1995). Social competence in the school setting: Prospective cross- domain associations among inner-city teens. W, 416-429. Lyman, R. D., Prentice-Dunn, S, Wilson, D. R., & Bonfilio, S. A. (1984). The effect of success or failure on self-efficacy and task persistence of conduct- disordered children. WW 516-519. National Center for School Safety. (1991). W W Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Justice. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Nuttal, E. V., & Kalesnik, J. (1987). Personal violence 1n the schools: The role of the counselor. Ianmalntflaunsehnaandfiexdapmentji 372- 375 Pepler, M. B. ,King, G. & Byrd, W. (1991). A social-cognitive based social skills training program for aggressive children. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds. ). . ... 'II.-.(pp361379) Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum. O' Donnell, J., Hawkins, J. D., & Abbott, R. D. (1995). Predicting serious delinquency and substance use among aggressive boys. 1onmal_of_Consulting_and WM), 529-537. Osofsky, J. D. (1995). The effects of exposure to violence on young children. Amensanlsxchalogrstamw), 782-788. ProthrOW-Stith. D. (1987). YiolenmmenfionLCunieulumioLadolescems. Newton, MA: Education Development Center. Prothrow-Stith, D. (1991). Deadlmnsequenees. New York, NY: HarperCollins. 105 Prothrow-Stith, D., & Spivack, H. (1992). Homicide and violence: Contemporary health programs for America' s black community. In R. L. Braithwaite &S. E. Taylor (Eds ),Hea1th.rssues_1n_the_blackcommumty(pp 132-143). San Francisco: J ossey-Bass Prothrow-Stith, D., & Spivack, H., & Hausman, A. J. (1987). The violence prevention project: A public health approach. WW We & 4), 67-69. Ray, W. J. (1993). Methods(4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. Roth, J. A. (1994). Understanding and preventing violence. Reseamhjnjfief. (NCJ-145645). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Short, J. F. (1990). W Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- 'Hall. Shure, M. B. & Spivack, G. (1988). Interpersonal cognitive problem solving. InR. H. Price, E. L. Cowen, R. P. Lorion, &J. Ramos-McKay. (Eds..) Emmeen ouncesmpmenfiommmmpraetifimm (pp. 69- 82). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Smith, V. E., Beals, G., Brant, M., & Annin, P. (1995, December 4). The lull before the storm? Newsweek. 40—42. Tate, D. C., Reppucci, N. D., & Mulvey, E. P. (1995). Violent juvenile delinquents. WW9), 777-781. Tolan, P., & Guerra, N. (1994a). Prevention of delinquency: Current status and issues. AW 251-273. Tolan, P. ,& Guerra, N. (1994b). WW2. Anemmnmlmampimefield. Boulder, CO: Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence. Wacgel.W- 13- (1989). WWW perspeet’me; Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Webster, D. W. (1993). The unconvincing case for school-based conflict resolution. WW2“). 126- 141. Wheeler, V. A., & Ladd, G. W. (1982). Assessment of children's self-efficacy for social interactions with peers. Wm), 795-805. 106 Winfree, L. T., Jr., Backstrom, T. V., & Mays, G. L. (1994). Social learning theory, self-reported delinquency, and youth gangs: A new twist on a general theory of crime and delinquency. WWI—25(2). 147-177. Wolfgang, M. E., Figlio, R., & Sellin, T. (1972). W Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Yeates, K. O., & Selman, R. L. (1989). Social competence in the schools: Toward an integrated developmental model for intervention. DexelopmemaLRerdew. 2. 64- 100. Zigler, E., Taussig, C., & Black, K. (1992). Early childhood intervention: A promising preventative for juvenile delinquency. AmedeanPsyehplogisLAlw), 997- 1006. "I11111111111118?