‘v V l 0 35:2, 01' E ; 7 ’§ ~I. 1' Ar 1 u x 5-: in“; tax-55¢. .,_; I 1?» ’vv. «:5 ’ v ‘ ’i . § “1 “,fifkflif‘l'} , ’( ‘ I“! "4:“: 5 '.‘t:ax .. 1-. _»:F‘1 xmsr *: r * 3* “5M?“ 1*». ”gays . Q oi: £11: ways ‘~ 9 30:1??‘1i9‘ WW," "7.- .1 11725 ’ ra‘ ’ ‘ l !‘ aw”. ,,< Um, r! guy-Ir \ \‘l 1.. . “:5 :13 a“: «2‘?- t % 33193 3‘? 1"” "(‘3 c' 'n-"" a J. ,. at. n. ‘ :‘u-‘x 5"? Ems?" ‘7‘” ,x 1 5'! 1: x3: 5 .1 x K"? V '43 2‘5 NIVERSITY LIBRARIES IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIILO II I I I IIII 3 1293 0140 This is to certify that the thesis entitled Construct Validity of Adolescent Alienation: Examining Age, Gender, School Environment, and Family Environment presented by Melissa Sue Huber-Yoder has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. Psychology degree in Mfm, Major professor 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACER RETURN BOXtomnaumbchockomnomywrm. TO AVOID FINES return on or baton duo duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF ADOLESCENT ALIENATION: EXAMININGthE, GENDER, SCHOOL ENVIRONMENT, AND FAMILY ENVIRONMENT BY Melissa Sue Huber-Yoder A.THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1995 ABSTRACT CONSTRUCT VALIDITY OF ADOLESCENT ALIENATION: EXAMINING AGE, GENDER, SCHOOL ENVIRONMENT, AND FAMILY ENVIRONMENT BY Melissa Sue Huber-Yoder 316 rural Midwest students (g = 742 with archival data) from grades seven through 12 completed questionnaires. The author devised measures of school and family alienation and empowerment. Alienation was a valid construct as measured by the author's scales (.53 to .60) and Dean (1961) alienation (.84). Alienation and empowerment were highly correlated within the same setting (5 = -.67 at home; E = -.91 at school). Two path models tested an ecological theory of alienation at home and school (family: 1‘} = 2.68, p=.612; school: 35} = 23.67, p - .257). Individual factors of age and gender were not useful in predicting alienation. Most environmental characteristics in each setting were moderate to strong predictors of alienation (r = .16 to .78). Alienation was small to moderate in predicting outcomes in the famdly or at school (5 = .19 to .33). Findings suggest that interventions should promote empowerment and focus on individual and ecological factors. Dedication Darin Yoder Jan & Robb, To my husband for his support, encouragement, endless trips to the library, whose powerful vision for rural youth empowerment provided the inspiration to complete this task. Rick Huber To my mother, and first psychology teacher, who embodies the art of active listening and taking another's perspective with great empathy. To my father, who taught me to ask difficult questions and not to settle for traditional answers when they seem unjust. Nate, & Kristin Huber To my brothers & sister-in-law who are the real talented siblings in my family. Robb, the carpenter, makes the world a more beautiful place to live, sharing his skills at work and with his family. Nate, the potter, will fill the world with inventive works as a skilled artist and scientist. Kristin, the nurse, shares my love of learning and keen desire to reform education. Georgia, Marv, Shelly, & Nate Yoder To my in-laws who welcomed me into their family and shared their support. Your courage and determination to live your own dreams has been an inspirational model of true success. Phyllis & Abe Roth, Joann & Mel Huber To my grandparents for their example of service to others and their constant encouragement of all their grandchildren. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author gratefully acknowledges several people who have been instrumental in the completion of this project: Current chair, Ralph Levine, for countless hours of careful review of many drafts, valuable feedback, a willingness to teach statistical software, and a strong motivation to convert the scientific community to confidence intervals. Current committee member Bill Davidson for his helpful comments and careful review of earlier drafts, skilled guidance for handling difficult statistical problems, and assurance that this project could be completed. Current committee member Jackie Lerner for her shared knowledge and enthusiasm for adolescent issues, support, and professional development guidance. Former committee member TomIReischl for his help with statistical software, insightful reading of previous drafts, and carefully documented comments provided encouragement and a clear direction for revisions. Former committee chair LaRue Allen for her enthusiasm and active listening that enabled me to translate my visions into a manageable project. Classmates and 818 colleagues, Becki Campbell, Lynn Breer, Holly Angelique, Bianca Guzman, David Canales-Portalatin, Hoa Nguyen for support and statistical guidance. Special thanks to Tina Tinskey and Brenda Vette for their invaluable administrative assistance in the data collection phase of this project. ii Table of Contents List of Tables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii List of Figures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Status of youth in Michigan and Nationally. . . . . . . 1 Outcomes Associated with Alienation . . . . . . . . . 3 Individual and Ecological Nature of Alienation . . . . . 8 Social Significance of the Problem. . . . . . . . . . 11 Overview of Possible Literature . . . . . . . . . . 12 Literature Review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 Age Level as an Individual Correlate of Alienation . . . . 12 Gender as an Individual Correlate of Alienation . . . . . 16 School Environment as an Ecological Correlate of Alienation . 21 Family Environment as an Ecological Correlate of Alienation . 26 Empowerment as a Contradiction to Alienation . . . . . . 30 Correlates of Empowerment . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 Outcomes Associated with Empowerment. . . . . . . . . 32 Operationalization of Measures. . . . . . . . . . . 34 Reasons for the Present Study . . . . . . . . . . . 38 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 Bivariate Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 Individual Correlates of Alienation and Empowerment . 40 Ecological Correlates of Alienation and Empowerment. . 42 Alienation Outcomes . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 iii Integrative Path Models Method. . Sample. . . . . . . . Design. . . . . . . . . Procedures Results . . . . . . Measures and Psychometric Properties. Reliability . . . . . . . Alienation Measurement Development School alienation & empowerment Family alienation and empowerment. 0 Construct Validity and Measurement Quality. Alienation and Empowerment Quality of Measurement Construct Validity of Alienation . . . Construct Validity of Empowerment. . . Discriminant Validity. . . . . . Methodological Issues. . . . . Potential Restriction of Range. . Bivariate Hypotheses . . . . . . . Individual Correlates. . . . . Ecological Correlates of Alienation Classroom environment. . . Family environment. . . . Alienation Outcomes . . . Integrative Path Models . Family Environment. . . . . . School Environment. . . . . . iv 44 48 48 49 50 51 51 51 54 54 55 56 57 59 62 64 66 66 71 71 73 73 76 79 80 81 83 Discussion Nature of Alienation . Nature of Empowerment. . Individual Variables . . Ecological Variables . . Summary . . . Recommendations. . . . Appendix Confirmatory Factor Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Appendix Environment Scales. Confirmatory Factor Environment Scales. Confirmatory Factor Scale . . . . . Confirmatory Factor Empowerment Scale . Intercorrelation of .Attenuation . . . Analysis of Classroom Analysis of Family Analysis of Dean Alienation Analysis of Alienation & Scales Corrected for Measures of Alienation . . Measures of Empowerment Measures of Classroom Environment. Measures of Family Environment. 87 87 90 91 91 93 93 98 99 100 101 102 105 106 107 108 Appendix J Measures of Involvement . . . . . . . . . . . 109 List of References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110 vi Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 List of Tables Negative Behaviors Associated with Alienation. . Positive Behaviors Associated with Lack of Alienation . . . . . . . . . . . . . General Individual Correlates Associated with Alienation . . . . . . . . . . . . . Relationship of Age to Alienation. . . . . . Relationship of Gender to Alienation. . . . . Relationship of School Environment to Alienation . . . . . . . . . . . Relationship of Family Environment to Alienation . . . . . . . . . . . . . Measures of Psychological Functioning Related to the Family . . . . . . . . . . . . Measures of Psychological Functioning Related to the Classroom . . . . . . . . . . Percentage of Respondents by Grade Level . . . Alienation and Empowerment Correlation Matrix. . Results of Hypotheses for Alienation and Empowerment . . . . . . . . . . . Parallelism Between Alienation and Empowerment in School and Family . . . . . . . . Alienation Outcomes by Grade Level . . . . Academic Outcomes by Grade Level . . . . . . Trend Analysis of Alienation Types as a Function of Grade Level . . . . . . . . . . . vii 13 17 22 27 35 36 49 56 63 65 67 69 70 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 Results of Hypotheses of Individual Correlates Results of Linearity Hypotheses of School Ecological Correlates. . . . . . . . Results of Correlational Hypotheses of School Ecological Correlates. . . . . . . . Results of Linearity Hypotheses of Family Ecological Correlates. . . . . . . . . Results of Correlational Hypotheses of Family Ecological Correlates. . . . . . . . Correlation Results of Alienation Outcomes in Family and School . . . . . . . -. . . Correlations for Family Alienation Path Model. Correlations for School Alienation Path Model. viii 72 75 76 78 79 80 83 86 Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure List of Figures Predicted Path Model for Family Empowerment and Alienation . . . . . . . . . Predicted Path Model for School Empowerment and Alienation . . . . . . . . . Construct Validity of Alienation and Empowerment . . . . . . . . . Final Path Model for Family Alienation Final Path Model for School Alienation ix 45 47 58 82 84 Introduction Status of youth in Michigan and Nationally Current demographic indicators suggest that youth have an increasing risk of adverse outcomes for their personal well-being and quality of life. A sizeable number of youth nationwide and in Michigan have poor outcomes in the areas of physical health choices, delinquency, and academic achievement. Physical health choices of youth have put some youth at risk for substance abuse and teen pregnancy. Nationwide, over 15% of teachers in public elementary and secondary schools during 1990-1992 reported student drug use as a problem, while nearly 25% reported student alcohol and tobacco use as a problem (U.S. Department of Education, 1992). Self reports among youth supported these teachers' perceptions. In 1991, 35% of youths aged 16 to 17 reported using alcohol and 21% reported cigarette use (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1993). In addition to choices about substance use, choices regarding sexual activity have lead to increased risk of early pregnancy. Births to teens without diplomas increased by 25% between 1980 and 1990 in Michigan (Kids count in Michigan, 1993) and by 16% nationally from 1983 to 1992 (Kids count data book, 1993). Substance abuse and teen pregnancy each appeared to affect nearly one quarter of youth. In addition to substance abuse and teen pregnancy, juvenile delinquency impacts many youth. The rate of juvenile delinquency has increased for Michigan and the nation. Violent crime arrests for Michigan youth rose 25.8% between 1980 and 1990 (Kids count in Michigan, 1993). This upward trend was larger for the nation. The national juvenile violent crime arrest rate for youth ages 10 to 17 increased by 48% from.1986 to 1991 (Kids count data book, 1993). Juvenile arrest rates increased from 1980 to 1990 for various offenses. .Arrests for l 2 murder increased 87.3%, weapons law violations arrests rose 62.6% and arrests for heroin/cocaine increased 713.8% (U.S. Department of Justice, 1992). Furthermore, for youth ages 15 to 19 in the United States, the violent death rate increased by 13% between 1983 and 1992 (Kids count data book, 1993). Increasing delinquency rates indicate a grievous problem.with serious consequences for youth in Michigan and the nation. Unfavorable school climates and poor academic achievement are also a problem impacting many youth. Inside the school setting many teachers have observed student behaviors that contribute to a poor school climate. Over 35% of teacher in public elementary and secondary schools nationally during 1990-1992 reported that student absenteeism.and tardiness were problems. Nearly 30% of the teachers reported that verbal abuse of teachers and physical conflicts among students were problems at school. (U.S. Department of Education, 1992). The achievement patterns of youth are also problematic. Nationally, 4% fewer teens graduated from high school in 1992 compared to 1983 (Kids count data book, 1993). While Michigan experienced 5.8% fewer dropouts from 1980 to 1990, several thousand students still withdrew from.school. In the 1991-92 school year, 20,032 (4.9%) high school dropouts were reported in Michigan (Kids count in Michigan, 1993). Many dropouts were also unemployed. For teens aged 16 to 19, 35,064 (6.2%) were not in school or the labor force in 1990 (Kids count in Michigan, 1993). Outcomes for students remaining in school were also unfavorable for a large group of students. Education statistics from the 1992-93 school year indicated that only 52.3% of Michigan's students were eligible for state-endorsed diplomas in math (Kids count in Michigan, 1993). High school dropouts, unemployment, absenteeism, and poor academic achievement are problems impacting many youth. These demographic statistics suggest that youth are estranged from society in many ways. Personal health, community safety, school attendance, and academic achievement were unimportant or unattainable for some youth. It is interesting to note that these outcomes have been 3 associated with youth alienation. Alienation has been associated with negative outcomes and lack of positive outcomes for individual youths, their health, academic outcomes, family life, and delinquency in their conmunities. A review of several studies suggested that alienation had a significant role in these detrimental outcomes affecting a number of youth. .Alienation may be seen as an important element of substance use, delinquency, and poor academic achievement. This paper will explore antecedents and outcomes of alienation with attention to both individual and ecological factors. The research findings from the literature are detailed further in the tables provided for the reader. Outcomes Associated with Alienation’ Alienation has been a concern of social scientists for several decades. The importance of alienation has been demonstrated by its alignment with problematic results for youth (See Table 1). Physical health behavior deficits have been correlated with alienation. .A small relationship was found between alienation and drug use (Jones, 1977; Owie, 1988), and alcohol abuse (Albas, Albas, McCluskey, 1978; Holmgren, Fitzgerald, & Carman, 1983). Early sexual behavior was significantly related to alienation from parents (Miller 8 Simon, 1974). Alienation was also related to psychological correlates. Relationships were found between general alienation and low self-esteem.(Warner & Hansen, 1970), anxiety (Warner & Hansen, 1970), depression and (Schimek & Milstein Meyer, 1975) suicide attempts (Kinkel, Bailey, & Josef, 1989). In addition to psychological outcomes, alienation has been significantly associated with behaviors that jeopardize communities, such as a history of conviction (Reimanis, 1974) and anti-social behavior (Schimek & Milstein Meyer, 1975). These behaviors suggest that the health and well-being of youth are jeopardized by alienation. ‘Throughout this paper, correlations are reported using effect size standards of magnitude based on the work of Lipsey & Wilson (1993). .nwoa auuuwhqi I I eaaom udooaou soda-usua- anodesn ao.vm .veo. + nn.I I u mad as an nuaom eonnoeaeh .Aoma. asoo doocon no“: usueceua< Aaron. cones: a Means: I I Muoaxed uneuueux coda-ae«de nuoaesn Ho.vm .moo. + vm. I u mad as an .mnaa. uoH>oa .aoon. anon Hoocon no“: veueeoaa< .onoa. sense: a ueeunz soduasnaaou agenda-a usuuasamxnai loses. mo.Im .oo.n -_.x «v as a: scannouooo nuaom Anon”. saunasox uceoneaou< uo>ox caounuaz u xoaacom acasacoo deacon noduaausdou massauem oauuuwcuwnn .npmac Hoo.Im .w~.v« n..x av we as quct no agendadaoo Inflow .mmma. ecundeox ueeonoHou< uo>ex :«oundax a uoaacom a: no.o emwaaou 2. 3... «8:3 :3: mo.vm Ho ceuo«>eooao .o». I auaaaoaAHeu. nusocsun uo euca>aou .coquQou uoc I u no“ a: vH.oH oouoa>coo nascea Lona”. odoum sums cao uao> auto” .vpoa. «acceded Ann. I auaadnaaaou. .Ho. I huaaanaAAOM. I I eudmceodunoso eoduocoddt Ho.vm .mpo. + .od. I u can an an on: case .HomH. anon undo u~e> sauna Amman. oazo Hoo.vwl pa unsound .uoquQou so: I u onm a: we no ma one as usuaou souu eouuneeud< ooao use» ouiva .vrma. :OSAm u usudaz mm. 0» ma. I «same .Hoo.vm anaeunnom .An.n~H cu m.a~ I .x ”new we «a nudaouua ouaousm can noduuaunuuan swan undo ume> maiua “mead. woman a .>e~«~m .Hoxcax I oo. I acauacOAHa suob .mm. I m cadauasx com we a: can: nosuv no oozed Hao«u«H0d u ”macaw nucocsun hudnuo>qca Ahead. nocou HocooAs I I Hocooan ocqxeduc Amman scans 0:3 nucoosun Amooav ovo. + ma. I u va a: a: now codua>duox .uonnob. c0«uae0«H< Hoocom so“: Hausa emauwo u .vanuoouudm .e0uosaom 84 I A SH 3 a: 3:81. 2:53 303 $833 3392 352:; 323 83: 8:300: 632 332 I II I .oaoadua> eeoouso. .oHnnwua> uouodooum. swam uoouum 2 cm x ensues: nocuo ousnmox noduacOHH< noduaasnom eocoueuom c0333.“: cud: cousaoouud uuoa>scom o>aummoz .H mannaunv 5 Since the presence of alienation has been associated with problematic behaviors, it is not surprising that alienation has been negatively correlated with beneficial outcomes at school and at home. Positive results at school have not been found when alienation is present (See Table 2). Low to moderate relationships were found between participating in activities at school and the lack of alienation (Burbach, 1972; Kulka, Kahle, & Klingel, 1982). Kulka, et a1 (1982) has suggested that a feedback loop exists between alienation and involvement at school. Similarly, a lack of alienation is also associated with academic achievement. Although alienation and grade point averages were unrelated for highly alienated students (Warner & Hansen, 1970), other researchers found moderate to large relationships between alienation and grade point averages among high school students in general. Burbach (1972) found moderate relationships between measures of alienation and grade point averages for high school students. Moderate to large relationships were also found between alienation and academic achievement among high school students (Hendrix, Sederberg, & Miller, 1990; Verma, 1990). .Academic achievement and involvement at school are positive outcomes associated with the lack of alienation. Positive involvement at home and in the community are also associated with the lack of alienation. Rode (1971) found that high school students' positive involvement with father and mother had a low association with the lack of alienation. Additionally, Calabrese & Schumer (1986) demonstrated that low alienation was moderately related to participation in community service. This positive relationship between involvement and reduced alienation is further characterized in Rafky's (1979) research showing that adolescents with higher political efficacy were less likely to exhibit expressive alienation. A.moderate effect size was found for this relationship. Additionally, Zimmerman (1990) found moderate to large effects indicating that participation was related to empowerment, and both empowerment and participation were H.s n.so N unsquson o.m s.ms H unsununod m.r m.or unsuieun «A mass: cu H.ca ¢.p> N unsuuunon ~.v~ a.mp A unsqun0d «.AA m.np unsuisuo m” sass: o” o.va o.Hm ~ unsuiunOQ o.nH n.ph H unsqunoa v.~d m.pp unsuIsua or. I auaadasdasu mm sass: 0 usququ soon I I ecuusddoauusm .Hsouc Ho.vm ava. + vow. In sou>usm auaasssou aoduaesua< asso suasvsun susuo cum momma. usescom s snsunsasu as. I auaaaosaaem .«pma nosnusm. I I nsscsnsauszod nauseous access code so.vm «so. + ms~.I In men an an sosus>< sasoa ovauo access and: "nuns scans nuNHIauod .mams. nuanusm I I lawns .usuuaz . amaze nauseous scone» nus: Ho.vm ovo. + oH~.I In non an a: sosus>< uaaom susuo nnsennsauszoa Hudson Hausa scsuo cuwflIzuoH A~>mac cosnusm or. I audaanswasu couuodsu asso .aoofl I I .asso. assasssauszod suesosus ”cocoa and: so.vm ovo. + mH~.I In new a: a: sumus>< uuaom susuo Hausaoom Hausa susuo cuwaucuon “whoa. compass as. I Nuaaansaasm ssaua>auos “was” compass. I I unassuuusosuuxm nnscnnsauszod nauseous Hoocon no“: Ho.vm «vo. + vn~.I I u new a: ma uo noses: Hoocom so“: Hanan sosuo cDNHInuoa “upmac combusm assus>suoa I I unflaosuusoauuxm less” .udussx s "aux. success» goons» so“; do.vm avo. + osH.I I u new we a: no usnssz nsscnssdussom asaoom Hausu scsuu cuNHIcuoa .mpma. combusm on. I audaunsaasu usququ asso assua>duos .HsmH I I usasoauuzosuuxm .asso. nnsenssauszod nucscsus Hoocos so“: ao.vm onv. + ~p~.I I u new s: we no ashes: Hausdoom Asusu scsuo cuuaIcuoa Aurmfl. compass I II I .s~ns«us> .sannaus> uouofiusum. swam uosuuu 2 cm : ssoouso. saunas: useuo susnssx acausnsunt nodusasnom sensuous: coausGUAH4 no xosq cue: pouedoonndtsuoa>scom o>auasom N manna or. I nunnnnannsn usunoosn ease Amman .csoo. Ho.vm woo. H pun.u I m man s: a: uessnssodsm noduscsnat anon ssussosnonsua: .ooon. unansssnn I I sesausnnasano no.vm amo.+ can. I n can a: s: a“ canusdnonunsm unsanszodau usuasusnonsue: Roman. amsnseann me. I aunannsnasn cannonsn case I I unsubsNAasono .noma .asso. Ho.vm omo. + ~H~.I I n can a: an an canusono«unsm soduscsau< ease nsusspanunspe: .oomfi. emansaann or. I aunannsnasn oeunoosn ease I me I m .HsmH anodes“ .noo. + vmo. In om” an a: sosns>s venom scene .asso. acausesaa< noocon can: vsusasuat “chad. cones: s nscnsz I I .cmmn .«anacm. snnom snuan an no.vm «an. + omm. In as an s: uasas>saco< owesuso< noduscsan< unsosum nauseous snsno can" Aommn. sens> I no.vm nucsnsn cut: .mno. + m-.I In ucses>ao>ca ssuunsom canew\nscuoz I .vsmH sedans> oscsunocm no.vm .oso. + noH.I In .nsusscomv >nouas>eH Anoan .aounnasxv nauseous cancoxnscumm new a: a: no«>snsm nesnsm snsom nodusesna< scene cuwaIcnoH .HpmH. scam scanusu:0nuaoo use annnscas> Arman Hoocos uo uncoon I I .nncaso s acunsmc Iunsn "noduaesnd< no.vm Hmo. + mo~.I In com as s: >osouuum Hsodunnom s>dnnsndxm nucsonsaoc< .mean. henna mo.vmimsn.~ IN nooaon sang I noozon moo.0II:Esm unses>uo>ed .Nomn .nsocnnx .sncsx assumes» s acnu Amman. mn~.o Isaac son an s: nunoan aunnns> .sxasx. canuaesna< s>on scans cn~nicuo Hsocqax a .sach .sxapx mo.vm nh~.~ In doocon snag I aoocos mmné Lassa .33 £353. 628. 5233:» s son» 83: pmn.o Issmx pow a: I: bases>no>an Inano .axnpx. conuaasnns sacs sauna guNHIguo nsoannx s .snaax .Ixnpx Lemon .nsaaqz s .ononnscsm snooze» I I .xanvcsmc noduscsn~< can: sunnsbss unszcaa .oman. nsaaax Ho.vu one. + «cm. In awn“ an a: scenes: uaaom scone .n> ucseunssou Hoocom n sen» anodesm s .onsnnsosm .xnnccs: I II I .s~ns«na> .sansans> nonuncsnm. snnm nosuum 2 on : ssoouso. ensues: nscuo ensues: sodusesddt soda-asQOm suesnsusm lu.ucoo. ~ ensue 8 associated with a lack of alienation. From this it is apparent that positive behaviors in the school, the family, and the community are more likely to be evident when alienation is not present. Within various settings, alienation is an outcome that is associated with undesirable outcomes for youth. Alternatively, the absence of alienation is associated with desirable outcomes for youth. Because alienation is linked with these outcomes, attention must be given to the various antecedents of alienation. Individual and Ecological Nature of Alienation Both individual and ecological factors have been construed as integral antecedents of youth alienation by researchers and theorists. This dynamic has been present throughout the history of the study of alienation. In the early development of the alienation construct, Seeman (1959) and Dean (1961) conceptualized alienation as a phenomenon influenced by both individual and situational factors. Bronfenbrenner (1974) also pointed out that many systems, including the family, work, and community, have a role in fostering alienation. This pattern of looking at macro—level and micro-level causes of alienation has been inherent in the discussion of youth alienation. Many individual factors have been researched for their impact on adolescent alienation (See Table 3). These personal variables include the youth's perceived ability (Mackey & Ahlgren, 1977) popularity with peers (Mohanty, 1984), socioeconomic status (Rafky, 1979), and parental occupation and education (Mackey & Ahlgren, 1977). In these empirical analyses, small relationships were found between alienation and most of the individual variables. Two exceptions were moderate relationships between alienation and youths' and lack of perceived ability (Mackey & Ahlgren, 1977) and lack of popularity among peers (Mohanty, 1984). mm mb.n mn.nm ooaanxscn smu ~o.s bv.nm nonon sonnnxm ucossocununo wma mm.n hw.nm ascoaunouonm nonsuasu I conusdsooo .mnmn .asesom. .pbmn. Hmm.um .mao. I n uconsm conussonad. suduusuaoud denuded a hogan: mm vm.m mv.hm ooaaaxscb omm hm.w po.pm nonsa Usaaaxm aunosmsocn nva mb.m H¢.vm asconnuouonm asconnom I ooo.um conuussuoo Amman .ansvom. .pemn. .mvo. + sma. u n uconsm doauscondd. uncoonoaoud nonmanfl a mason: «N n~.n mn.m~ omuno>o zonom new mn.m mo.s~ ouonosm hen pn.¢ ov.m~ smsno>s s>oo< ssocssoaoonsu I ooo.nm sunnnnm Amman .cusoom. “span. .Hvo. + Hmm. I n os>noonsm coauscoadd. sucsouoaoum nonmand w asxosz mm ms.m ¢H.mm omnns>o sonsm mum mm.n om.Hm omsno>¢ ucofioucsnuso mmH ww.m HN.mm smsno>s o>on¢ asnsuaso I moo.nm Rosanna Amman .cssusm. reams. .mvo. + mvH. n n oo>aoonom coauscoAHd. nud00s0a0dd. nonmanfl w Saxon: «N sn.p Nv.ov omano>m zonom new mm.m mm.mm ommno>< hudusmsocd bra Hm.m mm.vm omnno>s s>on< asconnom I ooo.nm sunnnnm Amman .cusosmc lbpmnc .mvo. + How. u n Uo>noonom coaumcoaad. nucsonoaond nonmacd a assoc: “oaosnns> Aoaosnns> oEoousoc I II I nouonosnmc ensues: swam avenue .2 am 2 ensues: nocuo coauocoadfl conusasdom ooconouom coauscodad and; cousaoosnd_sousaonnou assofi>aocu denuded n OHAUB 10 as I‘m .mmoh saunas coauscsaas H oao.I u n mom s: an oaeocooooaoom o>ansonmxm uncoouoaoud .mbmac hxusm a: I‘m .spmn .nansnsx I .ssnw unood gun: a span. success» + ~>N.I n n «m as so unansasdom coausdoaad. ease unaccouom .vmma. mucosa: no I‘m Amhma .acsonsx nudoosun I .msuw snows gun: a soap. unseen + ~m0.I u n ma so so muansasmom coauscsAHd. ansOdoosm “wand. zydeco: mam hm.q m¢.m~ noose» can: Nva Ho.m mm.m~ sooaaou assessedooasu I voo.um conuuosuu Amman .cseoume reams. .mvo. + ova. u n uconsm coauscoadd mucounoaou¢ nonmazfl w moses: new Hm.m v>.mm nooaon sane unssomcmnuns mma No.m vm.mm omsaaoo Hensuaso I conusosum Amman .cofioom. .bhmav mba.nm .hmo. u n uconsm coauscoaafl. nucsonoaond. nonmasfl a usxosz mam mn.s mm.sm nooaon none monomsuucn va mm.o mm.vm omsaaoo ascosnsm I moo.um conunosum Ammmfl .csfissmc Abpch .mvo. + men. I n ucsnsm conuscsnad. sncoonsHocd_ canvass s >sxosz ooa mm.¢ H>.m~ Usaanxmca omm ma.m Hm.mN nonca ouaafixm sea mm.v mm.m~ asconnsomonm nuscnuoascnsw I ooo.nm conuoasooo Amman .csEosm. .ppmnc .Nvo. + van. u n uconsm connocOAHd nucoonoaond nonmacfl s >oxom2 AQEoonso. I II I .nouonoonmv ensues: swam vacuum .2 am 2 ensues: nscuo coauscsadd coansasdom oocsnsuom lu.ucou. m snows 11 Within the social sciences there has also been attention to contextual variables in the construction of adolescent alienation. General societal factors theoretically linked to alienation have included the fostering of ambiguous social roles for youth (Young, 1985), breakdown of the neighborhoods, communities, and extended family (Bronfenbrenner, 1974), loss of social support, extreme mobility, and contradictory societal values for youth (Baumrind, 1987). These multiple facets of the community, school, family, as well as individual variables have been described as factors impacting the emergence of youth alienation. Social Significance of the Problem It is important that social scientists understand these empirical issues related to the development of youth alienation. This is important to adequately address the problem of adolescent alienation for the benefit of the alienated individuals as well as the communities in which they reside. These concerns arise from functional and philosophical foundations. Pragmatic considerations for society include questions about the role of the alienated youth. Their full participation in the community cannot be realized if they have dropped out of school, suffer from drug use, have low self—esteem, or lack motivation. Humanitarian considerations compel helping professionals to ameliorate individual suffering such as depression, powerlessness, and behaviors leading to health problems or attempted suicide. To better understand the etiology of alienation, it is important to compare the personality and environmental models of alienation. By understanding the influences in the development of alienation, social scientists can discern where intervention efforts are most likely to be effective. Additionally, community psychologists are admonished to go beyond preventing alienation and look toward promoting empowerment (Kieffer, 1984; Rappaport, 1981). Since alienation is contrasted with empowerment (Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988), this is well within the scope of 12 community psychologists. By systematically studying alienation and its juxtaposition, social change agents can try to reduce sources of adolescent alienation and cultivate seeds of empowerment within the environments that affect adolescents. Overview of Possible Literature A meaningful and comprehensive scope for this review of a vase body of literature included empirical studies of youth alienation obtained through computer searches using PSYCInfo and PSYCLit. Given the scope of the database, this discussion focused primarily on research published in English from 1968 to the present, therefore international research was limited. Unless specified otherwise, all research citations referred to studies conducted in the United States. Within this framework, citations were further limited to research in which adolescent alienation as an outcome variable was measured against predictor variables of age, gender, family environment, or school environment. Also included in this review were empirical studies focusing on alienation as a predictor variable for academic achievement, school attendance, and involvement with school and family. Literature Review Age Level as an Individual Correlate of Alienation Age has been often studied as a correlate of youth alienation (See Table 4). Among students, alienation primarily decreased with age in non-experimental pencil and paper studies that follow. Avi-Itzhak (1987) found that among Arab secondary students, younger students were more alienated than older students. Among students from private high schools, Rode (1971) also found that younger students were more alienated than older students. Jackson (1973) found that among 18-21 year old students, both younger males and females were much more 3 1 on. on me. I sensosnsn non >unnnnnnaom sameness unsennsnssns> .mrmn Iowan .aOsaosb. canuaesnat seasssn susscsnunsoes Ho.vm on.>I ax mmm s: a: so< no easom no ono nse> Naipn .vpmn. someone we. I nasasnans Innsn non hunnnosnnsm .oomn .sOsxosb. canusasna< ssnnssn mo.vm .n.. ...x c- «a a: Ina no onaom no uno naos nNIon .npan. consume so. I unsasnsnnssoo non >unnnnsnnsm .comn .eonxosn. genusasnn< no.vm ps.m I.~x Ne s: an so< no snsom no sense uno nms> nquH .npan. coaxomn Anuaosoannso ow oo.m vv.~I mHIhn Amman snncs>sfi can .suesnno ow on.n ~a.nI snImn .nsnsnu s anseosuuoo. Uncnno cunssc an ~n.m nn..- .nInn conuINncmononno nausea .nuaaanonunua I I ow ov.m ov.mI NnInn n~c0nnsm asnOOno cuso>c .mponv mo.vm nmo. + omn.o In out can acnusesnn< Hanson sono nus» an on an emaunnsm s .usoenz..nsssnw an. I nunnnnannsm .~>on cosnnsmv I I suscnnsnnssoo mucousun Hoocun can: mvo. + omo.I In new we a: ns>sn scone noocom can: nanm sosno cuwnIcuon .mpmn. cosonsm I I Aswan .unuuox . naoz. nucouzun nooaon can: «vo. + ovo.I In new a: a: Hs>sn susnw nnscnnsnnssom nanUOm nsnsm susno cuwnIcuon Insane nosnnsm op. I aunnnnsnnsn nonnoasn case Insmn I I .csso. suscsssnnszoo nucsosun Hoocon can: 0.0. + nvo.I In new a: s: ns>sn scans ususnoom Munoz scans cunnInuon “whoa. cosonsm .anmnidasessm no woman .npmn I I .cosnsv:noucs>cn noocos >nsocoosn bsn< Ho.vm on". + nvv.I In on m: s: asssn susno genusuosoxm Hoocom suescsun sosno cum Arson. escnunIn>< I II I .snnann~> nononssnm. .snnonna> saoousoc snnm uosnnm 2 on x snsnss: nscuo snssssx canusasnnt nodusnsoom socsnsnsx conuscondd on saw no adcsdonusaom v sxnnaunv 314 no.vm noo. n pmn.I In mom Ga Ga 004 cannns> usasunocm Amman .counncoxv snsom noduscsnnt sucsusun suano aunnIauon .Hhmav ovum Sim $6 I .x No NC a: s04 on. 0» mp. I nodaonnnn non >unnnosnnsm Damonnsn uncannvanw3bm “mean .momn .aOnxomb. nodumcwnad no snsom no nouns susscsnonscc: uno nus» «NIpn “whoa. canxumb 36a 3.? .x omw MC NS 004 cm. on mp. I nonsonosn non >unnnnsnnsm snsonnsn nesesnsos Innsm Ampmn .oomn .conxosb. canumesnn< no easom ha nsHsson sussosnonsues ono nss> -Ihn .vrmav coaxomb snnm ausnnm 2| .Innanna> nouonusnm. Omanflox MOBUO lonnmnn~> seoouso. snssssz canuscsna< cannonsoom socsnsnem .u.ucoo. v canoe 15 alienated on specific dimensions than their older counterparts. Younger males felt more powerless than older males, while younger females were higher on self—abasement than older females. Jackson (1974) generally replicated these findings for 17-22 year old students. Once again, younger males felt more alienated on powerlessness, while younger females were higher on self-abasement than older females. Burbach (1972) found similar effects for an inverse relationship between alienation and age. Among rural high school students, Burbach (1972) found negative correlations between age and several measures of alienation, including Dean's societal powerlessness, Neal & Rettig's social powerlessness, and Burbach's high school powerlessness. These studies supported adolescent alienation decreasing with age. .Although research has provided support for the belief that alienation decreases with age, two other studies have contradicted these findings. In Jackson's (1974) study discussed above, older females were more alienated on valuelessness than younger females. This contradictory finding suggests that older females were more alienated in relation to perceived social norms and guiding values. Perhaps this can be attributed to the young women's observations of external circumstances and changing roles of women in society. These external appraisals seemed to measure a construct distinct from alienation based on internal assessments of one's power and self-concept. This suggests that researchers should attend to distinctions between alienation based on internal and external appraisals. .A second contradiction was found in an exploratory study analyzing the contents of 11 to 18 year old students' speech. Gleser, Winget, & Seligman (1979) reported a positive correlation between age and social alienation and personal disorganization. The sample for this study was composed of summer program participants, mental health clients, and juvenile delinquents. This population may be quite different from the previous studies conducted among typical junior and senior high school students. The seemingly atypical positive correlation between 16 alienation and age for this population may attribute to underlying alienating experiences for these youth that increasingly result in more contact with the mental health and juvenile justice systems. Unlike the previously mentioned studies, it is not clear that the measure of alienation used in this research is based on the constructs delineated by Dean or Seeman. This contradictory finding may be due to differences in the sample population, the Operationalization of alienation, or more likely due to sampling error resulting from a small sample size. These studies, focusing on students between the ages of fourteen and twenty-two, suggest that alienation, especially powerlessness, generally decreases with age among typical students. Not to be dismissed are the diverse results suggesting that differences in alienation may be found among students involved with the mental health or juvenile justice systems. Researchers may need to make distinctions between various types of alienation. While considering these differences, most studies are supportive of the idea that alienation decreases as adolescents become older, have greater access to resources, and gain more independence. One of the main objectives of the author was to determine how much overlap existed in various measures of alienation. The hypothesis that various measures of alienation were unrelated was considered and discussed for its implication about the nature of alienation as a construct (Refer to discussion section). Gender as an Individual Correlate of Alienation Gender has often been assessed in relation to youth alienation with mixed results (See Table 5). Males were shown to be more alienated in some studies while females were more alienated in others. Various types of alienation were measured which may be partially responsible for the apparent differences. Studies that found males to be more alienated 117 ”Q I m .osunoosn no: I «x ucsssnsosIann we. I assesses. Inns» non auannsnnsm so. I essannsnnsson non >ununnannex osusesnnm snos sons: .oomn Hoo.vm pm.o~ I_.x .aonnosn. canuscsnn< ssussusnonsuas nnscnnsnnszod can we a: neocsu no ensom no uno nss> onIon Amend. conxosb .ommn .nsnnn: s .unsnnsosm snoocos couscsnnslsnoe nonmm .xnnoesz. acnusasnnt can: cannons» unsscna .ommnc nonnnx Ho.vm .nmo. + moo.I I n nwmn noneso .s> neseunseoo noocom n aonn anodesm s .onsnnscsm .xnncas: .aomn .nsnsno u ensconuuow. mu oo.n mn.mI ensesn canusnnasonosno ususesnns mnoe nonsm mm sn.~ ~o.nI sass asconnsm mono Aswan. no.vm .onn. + man. I n nsscso sac nonuscsnn< nanoom nos» on on nn sung: auaonnsm s .usucnz_.nsosno .oomn .nsssno s uneconuuoo. cousconns once nsnsasm ow rm.n mv.~I snmaon acnusnncsonOnno I n: I m mu mm.~ n~.mo ease AseOsnsm nuno Aaron. .snn. + noo. I n nougoo can nonuaasnn< Hanson nus» on on nn nouns assunnsm s .usoanzc.nsnsnu up. I case an causesnns snoe nonmesm s: vo.vr snsesn osunoosn >unnnosnns¢ a: I m .nn I: ao.~p onus Insmn .osunoosn no: I n nsccso .esso. canuscsnat nunsosus susno cum Amman. annnsm s snsnnsnsu on. I aunnnnsnnem ohm va.» pm.om snsasn Amwmn cosnnsm. couscsnns smoe nonsssm nmw om.p mo.om sass snsanssansson nucsosun noonon can: «so. + Ono.I I n nsccso noocom can: Hanna susno cumnIcuOA Anson. coannsm o>~ ps.~ oo.o snassn ususasnns mnos ssnmm mam no.~ pp.m same Aswan .OAuusm a assz. nucscsun noocon cone no.vm nvo. + own. I n nsucso assasssnnsxom HsnOOm nsnam scsno cumnIcuon Laban. cosnnsm ohm no.~ Ho.~n snsesn .nomn couscsnns Mnoe nsnsm now no.n o~.~n onus .csso. onsasnsnnszon nuasssun noocon can: «so. + one. I n nounso asusnoom Hanna sosno zuwnIcuon .«nmn. cosonsm I II I Asnnsnns> nouUnusnmc .snomnns> saoousov snnm uusnnu 2 am : snsnss: nscuo snsnss: acnusasnnd cannonsoom suesnsnsx conusconfld on noodom no macncOHusHom m canes 218 “0&NGOHHO ONO—B ”GHQ: ”a I m cannns> usasunocm Amman .counnaex. nueevsus .30. n «8.- I m «an In .3 nouaso Snow 8323? scans 53133 :5: seem cannnsmun vov as o.“ sasaen no suns no unonsn osusasnns swoa nonessm non s: o.n sass Inns» "nannsasna< no.vm nmo. + pvn.I I n nsoeso s>nsssnoxu suceonsnout .ahmn. >xnmm ma oo.n no.ov sasssn ususcsnns mnos nonsm «m on.m mv.on sass .ohan .ncnsnsx snvnn no.vm on”. + cow. I n nsusso a nose. nodusesnn< .sssnno aonn sucsosum .voon. >uasco: .90. I Munnnnunnenm assesssnscnso I .ppmn .conana< ususesnns mnoe modem s asxoax_ canusasnn< nauseous no.vm nmo. + men. I n com a: as nsvcsu scone coon was nus .ppoH. nsnonc< s >sxomx Ace. I aunnnnsnnsn. unsasocsnuns Hansunsu ususesnna snma nonmesm I Arron .csnonct nuasosus mo.vm mno. + con. I n oom so a: nsuaso s asxosx. genusasna< eusno anon can can «span. csnuHc< s >sxom: .oo. I munannsnnsn. csusesnns snos nonmasm >unusnsoen unconnsm I no I m I .nbmn .esnonc< suescsun .wmo. + ado. I n oom so an nsoeso s asxosxv :Onusasnn< susno anon ocs cum .hpanv conuncx a honest an a: m.on sonaasn Acnmn .snsos couscsnns snos nonaesm so a: n.o sense genuscsnns ao>esq. nauseous noocon can: Amman. scosnnsz no.vm .vmo. H nnm. I m nsocsw acnusesna< nancsn nus» unnnm .cnunom .3» .snnoasq so. on me. ususEOnns I socsn >unnnnsnnsm snoa nsocso czocxca Anson .ooon noo.vm mm.vn I_~x .aonxosn. cenuscsnn¢ noussosnonsccs nnscnnsnssns> mmn an ac nsoasw no snsom no cno nss> -Io~ .vnmnc cOoxomn couscsnna snoa sons: 809mm 3.3 I .x as. on m... suscnnsnssns> I suesn >unnnosnnsm “mean .oomn mo.vm vo.v I ma .conxosb. cOnuscsnn< nouszosnunsocs asscs:0n< own as an neceso no snsom no uno nae» onIFH .vran. aOnxusb I II I asnnsnns> nouoncsnm. .snnenns> ssoouso. snnm nosnnu 2 cm x snssas: nseuo ensues: aOnusnsna< nodusnsnom soesnsnsx is.ucoue m manna 19 than females are discussed first. Hendrix, Sederberg, & Miller (1990) found males to be more alienated than females on a continuum of commitment & alienation when comparing seniors from Midwest suburban schools. On Dutt & Kureshi's measurements of alienation; including despair, disillusionment, unstructured universe, narcism, psychological vacuum; Mohanty (1984) found males to be more alienated than females when comparing students from Orissa, India. Jackson (1973) found 18-19 year old undergraduate males to be more alienated on the powerlessness dimension of the DJ scale of alienation than female undergraduates. Simdlar findings for powerlessness were reported by Jackson (1974) for males among 17-19 year old undergraduates and 20-22 year old undergraduates on valuelessness. Among ninth and tenth grade public school students, Mackey & Ahlgren (1977) found males to be largely more alienated than females on the guidelessness dimension of an alienation scale based on Seeman's constructs. Burbach (1972) found males to be higher on Neal & Rettig's social powerlessness and on Dean's measure of societal powerlessness. Males were also more alienated than females according to Rode (1971) who used a shortened version of the Keniston alienation scale with private school students from grades 10 through twelve. Among white 11 to 18 year olds, males were found to be more socially alienated in their speech as measured by content analysis in an exploratory study by Gleser, Winget, & Seligman (1979). Across several studies, males were more alienated than females in several age categories on measures of alienation, including powerlessness and valuelessness. In contrast to findings of more alienation among males, several other researchers reported higher alienation among females. Rafky (1979) found a higher rate of expressive alienation, such as vandalism and confrontations at school, among adolescent females. Among first year junior high school students LaTorre, Yu, Fortin, & Marrache (1983) reported higher alienation among females compared to males. Among ninth 20 and tenth grade public school students, Mackey & Ahlgren (1977) found females to be much more alienated than males in regard to cultural estrangement, and more alienated in terms of personal incapacity. Burbach (1972) also found female tenth to twelfth graders to be more alienated than males when compared on the dimension of high school powerlessness. Calabrese & Seldin (1986) reported higher levels of alienation, as measured by Dean's (1961) scale, among ninth grade females. Gleser, Winget & Seligman (1979) found higher levels of social alienation for females, as compared to males, among black 11 to 18 year olds. In these instances, researchers were able to demonstrate higher levels of alienation among females on some measures of alienation. It is important to note the use of multiple measures of alienation in these gender comparison studies. Important findings regarding alienation can vary widely depending on the type of alienation, and there may be interactions with other variables. To demonstrate this effect, the following studies from above are highlighted. In Rafky's study (1979), males were more likely to be rebellious and females were more likely to be alienated. In this study rebellion was linked to action taken to improve a situation of powerlessness while alienation was aligned with inaction. This dimension points to not only a difference in types of alienation but also to potential differences in self-efficacy as well as participation. In contrast to the inaction of the females in Rafky's study, another analysis (Calabrese & Schumer, 1986) conducted in the late 1980's contended that high school females' alienation levels decreased as they were particularly responsive to an involvement in a volunteer community service project. Although the types of involvement mentioned in these two studies use varied scales, it is possible that a difference between the earlier females' inclination for inaction and the later females' responsiveness to active involvement can be attributed to cultural maturation effects as the roles of women have moved from passive to more active and assertive. In considering the assessment of 21 alienation, gender differences may be masked in measurement issues and in interactions with other variables such as ethnicity. Referring back to Gleser, Winget, & Seligman (1979) of 11 to 18 year old students, race appeared to be a factor in outcomes for each gender. Among black students females were more alienated, but among white students male students were more alienated. When comparing male and female alienation it is difficult to make strong general statements about the role of gender in fostering alienation. Males and females from various ages, ethnic groups, and types of schools have been compared using a variety of alienation measures. Without replication of studies with similar variables and measurements of alienation it is difficult to make assertions about the role of gender as a single variable. In summary, research on individual differences related to alienation has suggested the following: First, a variety of factors may be responsible for fostering alienation among youths. Second, there may be problems of alienation construct validity if global measures of alienation do not accurately measure alienation. Finally, alienation may not be a unitary construct as measured by multiple instruments reviewed in this text. School Environment as an Ecological Correlate of Alienation In addition to personal variables such as age and gender, environmental variables, such as the school climate, also appear to have a role in the development of alienation (See Table 6). For example, some of the differences showing higher alienation among females have been attributed to the masculinization of the junior and senior high school environments (Calabrese & Schumer, 1986; Calabrese a Seldin, 1986; LaTorre, Yu, Fortin, & Marrache 1983). A.more in-depth look at dimensions of the school environment can provide clues about alienation. 222 Shine e.mmlumsuncou no. mmm.In nsenm Ennnsncounso .AnOnnsndosn on cOnusnsUnncoo. .nnsen00n< "uncounosnd .npan .no: u .nnsunu .nszonnn3. >oonoson Ann. I aunnnnsnnsn. nasssnsnocncss: .mmmn .nsssnox. snneesOnunsso anoocom I nnsnocncss: mv a: a: nonueoo nnnsm sosunuu< nnnsm can: means» 302 “Neon. >0: 36.31 n .mnIIumasncoo no. mmm.In nmcnu AnnOnssnoosn .mh. I munnnnsnnsn. sns mosanuen .unnono. unsannsnnssom .socsnucnz .Ennnsnvounsu Anson .50: s .nnsunm Amman .nsnsnox. “snouOnosno .nszonnnse >oonoscn snnaeaonunsso nnoocom I nnsconsnnszom mv a: an nonuaoo nnnsm sosunuu< nnnsm can: asnnsb 3oz Anson. >0: Anson nunonu I I s ananazc samenno Amman unooaom no.vm non. + mnv.I In mv sq so nseOnuaunesono .nsnsnox. acnuaesnn< can: assnsb 3oz Anson. >0: s nonunsm or. I nunnnnannsn men as m.on sussnnm usunonsn ease snoocon can: ns>sn mo.vm mmm a: p.pn Unnosa unsanssnEnoz I .nman sus>nnn can unnnso .osunOan uoc I n "noocon no sd>9 .essoc canumcsnnt sonn suesosuu snsssm .npomn. snsnnsnso or. I nunnnnannsn men a: n.m~ sus>nnm usunoosn asso snoocon can: ns>sH mmm s: n.p~ ennnsm genusnonn I .nwmn sus>nnd can ennnsd noo.vm .osunoosn no: I n "noocon no saha .csso. genuscsnn< Bonn nauseous snsesm “organ. snsnosnsu up. I >unnnnsnnsn s: I m men s: ~.mn sus>nnm osunonsn case nnoocon can: .ussnoaon no: I n mmn a: n.- onnnaa .nsmn su~>nna sea onnnsa cOnumcsnns nsuoa “noozon no en>a .caoo. canus=0nn< EOnn uncanny» snnfism Lehman. ononnsnsu usuacsnns Amman noozon snos nucsmsun nannw - vs. om.m nannm .cosnsu:<. >noucs>EH >nsucoosn none eonn no.vm an. + mnm. I n vn so. nn.m anon: acnusuosnxm noocom nauseous sosno cum Arman. escnunin>< en mm. om.~ sonsn nn or. no.m annuss .mpmn noonun I s: I m s an. oo.n nnmen .eonnsuc4. >nouas>en ansveoosn aunt sann Arman. xscnunIn>< .pmn. + mnn. I n snnm unsannonam acnusuosdxm neocom nauseous svsno cum I II I .snnanns> nouUnusnmc .snnsnns> ssoouso. snnm nosnnm 2 on x snsnssx nseuo snsnss: genusasnnt canusnsQOm soasnsnsm connsconnd on udoEdonn>cm noocom no oncnconusnom _w nannauhv 223 nanuonnso an ~n.ohn Iucssssnos son onusnasesc s: mo.>mn Iueoessnos son nanoonnao so mm.mon Inassssnos can: snunncsssc s: n~.~mn Inasessnos can: Enron .>o= s .nnsonu Amman .nsnsnox. I I .nPJOnnnE moonoson snnsccOnunsao no.vu «'0. + pnn. I n nvn nonucou nnnsm sosunuu< nnnaa nuasosun can: nanase .Nomnc ennssnm no.vo mmn. + now. I n ucsEnnOncw ucsosumi .smon .nsnsnoxc no.vm mmn. + wmv. I n snnmccOnussso sunn nnsum mv as so noocos no snnm sosunuu< nnnsa snoocon can: nnpan. >0: a noonnsnam npsmn .no: s .nnssnm roman .nansnox. I I .nszonnnx. >oonoson snnsc:0nunsso no.vm non. + an.. In m. a: a: nonncou nnasa scannnna nnasa anooaon eon: Inpon. >0: n nounnanux mnm.Iw_ o.nnInm~nncou no.v mom.In nuann .ms. I annnnnmnnon. annnsnoounso unscsnsnsnoz .ucsEsosocsnno .mnon .>om s .nnsonm Amman .nsnsnoxc “uncuonosno .nszonnnz. moonoson snns:20nunsso snoocom I anscnnonanoz m. In an nonnaoo nnasa ouannnna nnnsn can: nouns» 3s: insane no: I II I nonmmnna> nononuonns noannna>Immoon=oc sunm uosnnu 2 am I ensues: nscuo onsssox cos-con? canusnsnom eccensnsm AU.uCOUV 0 OHQMB 24 The size and geographical setting of the school are one facet of the environment related to student alienation. A large relationship was found between the geographic setting of the school and alienation. Students from the rural schools were more alienated than students from the urban school (Avi—Itzhak, 1987) which the author attributed to differences in social and political climates between the two settings. The size of the school was also a predictor of alienation. Large correlations were found to suggest that students from schools with a larger student enrollment and staff size were more alienated (Rafalides & Hoy, 1971) although these findings were not supported by Ayi-Itzhak (1987) in Arab secondary schools. These findings suggest that students can feel alienated in school settings that provide them fewer opportunities, such as rural schools, or in settings that are large or impersonal and leave students feeling isolated. Within the general environment of the school, the openness of the climate and the control orientation has been linked to student alienation. Students were less likely to be alienated in the more open schools as reported by Hartley & Hoy (1972). Similarly, Rafalides & Hoy (1971) reported that schools with a more custodial, rather than humanistic control orientation, had pronounced levels of alienation. Hay (1972) supported these findings in reporting custodialism.as a predictor of normlessness, powerlessness, and meaningless in multiple regression. Additionally, when students from.public and private schools were compared, the private school, described as more controlling than the public school, had more sizable levels of alienation than the public school on the dimension of isolation (Calabrese, 1987b). However, in this same study, students from the public school reported notable levels of alienation on the normlessness dimension. Calabrese (1987b) suggested that students from the private catholic school were given more guidelines, so there were clear norms established in the controlling environment. These studies suggest that school environments facilitated alienation if they were too highly controlled or isolating for students. 25 However, the school environment was also alienating when guidelines were unclear. .A healthy school environment can be described as one having a balance between establishing clear rules while retaining openness in the school and humanistic regard for students. In addition to these general characteristics of the school environment that foster alienation, a lack of fit between individuals and their environments was found to exacerbate alienation. Agreement between administrators and students regarding type of control and level of control was important. Additionally, agreement between students' developmental need for control and experience of control was necessary. Disagreement between students and school staff has signified higher levels of alienation. In one example (Shearin, 1982) eighth and ninth grade students in schools with low agreement between students and staff, in regard to pupil control orientation, scored higher on alienation than students from.schools with high student-staff agreement. Another example supports the importance of fit between the school environment and individual control ideologies. Students in a controlling environment with an intrinsic motivational style were less engaged (Boggiano & Katz, 1991). In the typical junior high school setting students had decreased opportunities for control although their developmental need for control increased. This created a motivational deficit for the students most advanced in their developmental need for autonomy (Eccles, Miller Buchanan, Flanagan, Fuligni & Yee, 1991). Having a match between students' developmental level and school environment was pivotal. Case studies of precocious junior high students pointed out that ill-fitting educational pace was responsible for alienating bright students. Bright, but uninterested students were seen as a problem. An alternate approach was developed which stated that the level of education was too elementary for such bright students that in turn lead to their disengagement in the classroom. Moving these students into advanced studies in an honor college appeared to remove the problem of alienation and their lack of acceptance by peers. These 26 otherwise disenfranchised students were quite successful in their new environment (Stanley, 1975). Overall, the degree of "fit" between student desires, developmental level, and school environment was a critical factor in reducing alienation and promoting positive student outcomes. Overall, the size, structure, setting, openness, and control orientation of the school environment were important variables to predicting assessing youth alienation. Family Environment as an Ecological Correlate of Alienation The family has been identified as a source of adolescent alienation. One theory states that this is due to the bureaucratization of the family in which parents are executives and children are products in a dehumanizing environment (Young, 1985). Previous research provides some support for the belief that the structure of the family can facilitate alienation as noted in table 7. Some elements of the structure of the family have been found to influence the alienation of the children while other components have not been found to be related to alienation. One—parent versus two-parent households had no noticeable relationship to youth alienation (Calabrese, 1988) and neither did the type of relationship that youth held with significant adults (Galbo, 1983). However, the birth order did appear to be a factor in alienation with a large correlation found between the two variables. Calabrese (1988) found older children to be more alienated than the later-born children. Further aspects of the quality of the family life have been related to alienation. Rural students in seventh, ninth, and eleventh grades that had been referred to a mental health center for interpersonal problems at home and school were shown to have higher alienation scores on the dimensions of meaninglessness and closemindedness than their peers (Gladding, 1977). The correlations between these variables were large. However, a moderate correlation revealed that the students not referred to the mental health center had 2'7 mn an n.av ennEOn en so n.mv snse nanucoo mn we «.mm snseon an a: m.v¢ snss susnosansuan mn a: m.mm snsssn en a: «.pm snne >50:0us< .vmmn .unsnum n 5328 .nonncoo 2. I 32358 .cooxv nuasecOnn>cs osunoo0n esso sunsos oaso> I I nsoeoe0us< .nmon ansnoIsnoonz cos no.vm men + omv. I n .n> oannnOnucou .csso. canuscsnn< sncsosun >unnns>nca nwmmn. curse on. I Nunnnosnnsn munasssoo uesnso snoenn osunonon esso ocsnocm 302 ansussz no I m .n> ucsnao snnsoo Anoan cannonsn s Eonn .oounoosn no: I m ov a: so snsuosnum >nnesm .esoo. eOnuscsnn< nauseous sosno cum Amman. snsnosnsu an. I annnnn-nnsn annazesoo osunoosn ease ocsnocm 302 ensunsz I I .nson cannonsn s acnn no.vm mvn. + mmm.I I n ov a: so nsono cunnm .easo. cOnuacsnnt sucsosun sosno cum .moonc snsnnsnsu I II I .snnsnns> nouonosnm. nonnsnns> sEoouso. snnm nosnnm 2 am : snssssx nscuo snsnss: eonuscsnnt cannonsnom suesnensm conuscondfl ou ucofidonn>cm annash no ancnoonusnom b vanes nl no.vm ano. n mmo. I mm mN mm mm I~N OF HN HH osusasnns son sonanonna can: nnnnu usuaasnna son sonaaonna can: n>om .snnann~> seconso. “swan .nsaooo. acnussns>m nesnsm no. I >unnnnanno¢ aonnns> noun: nonnanna> nouonoonmv «noon. canuscsnnd esso snocn Bonn nucsosum insane nnassx s .noanm .Izcnm no.va~mo. I no..- I n unnnw\nsznoz no.vm mmo. + n.m.- I unnno\nocn~n nl mom .vomn .nsnsscom. aOnusson>nocn no socsunooot I n0n>snsm unsnsm censnss oscsunocm Amman .cOunncox. snsom cenusesnn< nucsosun Guano zuwnicuon «noon. Guam 283 no.vm,oso. + man. I n unnaw\nsanoz no.vm one. + an. I annzoxnsanan 54 now .vomn .nsnsmcomv nonuaoo I >noucs>en n0n>scsm uaonmm cennns> osesunocm «noon .cOunncoxv snsom canusesnnt suasosun ouano annnIznon .npon. soom HI no. n su.o- I nl moo. H nnm. I nl moo. H arm. I Nnn om Nnn on Nnn on H nuasosun sucsnnOI nucsosun nucsnnOI sucsosun suesnnUI Ansnnsnsn nsuaso cunssc nausea on unnomsnv secs nu nmnnnsonnnnu nsconnsnnsucn nusescont unscosocnesnOnu unscnnsnoaneasx nsnsonnsn usnaoc< no snmom snoenm mono nss> onI~n nsnsm Aphan. oanuuano s: I m .osunonsn no: I n nm sunsoa unsunnncOnn gun: nananconnansm .vpmn .asxosz. unsannsns< canuscsnn< nasonsnoo< unanasn noocon can: Amman. canoe ounm uoonnu 2| 2: .snnanna> nononssnn. snsnms: nszuo nonnsnns> seoousoc ensues: canusesnnt eonusnsoom suesnsnom no.0d00. b wanna 29 higher scores on the aloneness dimension of alienation. It was assumed that these students were not having problems with their families, unlike their peers who had been referred to the mental health center due to family or school problems. These findings suggest that problems in the family may be related to some forms of alienation, although some experience of alienation may not be atypical. Since the non—referred students had higher scores on the aloneness dimension of alienation, there may be feelings of alienation that are a normal part of the adolescent developmental process. Other assessments of the quality of the home life were found to be related to alienation. Students scoring high on alienation reported having lower esteem for their parents (Sinha, Singh, & Kumari, 1988) and the correlation between these variables were very large. These studies suggest that there are differences in the family environments of alienated youth, although the direction of the influence is unclear. Structural components such as single-parent were not related to alienation, but the quality of the relationships with parents appears to be an important element. The controlling nature of the family has also been studied for its impact on youth alienation. Overall, higher levels of control in the family have been associated with alienation, just as it has been in the school environment. Conspicuous levels of alienation have been reported in family environments characterized by control rather than autonomy (DeMan, 1982). Rode (1971) reported that alienation was more salient with controlling mothers and fathers. Rode (1971) also reported large inverse relationships between alienation and both mothers' and fathers' acceptance of youths' individuation. The role of the controlling family climate is an apparent factor in youth alienation. In summary, some aspects of the family structure, the controlling climate, and the parent-child interaction have been found to relate to the development of adolescent alienation. 30 Empowerment as a Contradiction to Alienation Community psychologists are admonished to go beyond preventing alienation and look toward promoting empowerment (Kieffer, 1984; Rappaport, 1981). One alternative that has been offered to alienation is empowerment (Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Empowerment encompasses more than an absence of alienation. It has included self—efficacy, positive self-concept, social support, critical awareness and understanding, and participatory competence (Kieffer, 1984; Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Empowerment has been described as containing elements of positive self-concept or self-competence, analytical understanding of the socio-political context surrounding oneself, and the formation of individual and group resources for socio-political action purposes. The notion of participatory competence (Kieffer, 1984) includes the "cultivation of individual and collective resources for social and political action (p. 31)." This includes a behavioral dimension as well as a psychological dimension. Action cannot coexist with the inaction inherent within alienation. Rafky (1979) illustrated this point. In that study alienation was inversely related to rebellion. Rebellion contained an element of action and a desire to have an influence to make things better. The dimension of action is an important contradiction to alienation. Engagement and involvement have been used in contradiction to alienation. These positive outcomes have been facilitated by proper person-environment fit in the school setting (Boggiano & Katz, 1991; Eccles, Miller Buchanan, Flanagan, Fuligni & Yee, 1991). For these reasons it is important to assess the environmental impact on the development of empowerment in addition to alienation. Expanding studies of alienation to include dimensions of empowerment is one avenue to build on previous research. 31 Alienation, as construed by Seeman (1959), can be seen as a contrast to empowerment. Powerlessness, as a component of alienation, was described as a belief that behaviors could not produce desired outcomes. Similarly, meaningless was depicted as a low expectancy that future outcomes of behavior could be made. These dimensions typify a sense of hopelessness and futility in influencing outcomes. This has been confirmed by other research indicating that a lack of mastery and control was a key element in the development of alienation (Wilkerson, Protinsky, Maxwell, & Lentner, 1982). Conversely, theories of empowerment have included elements of power, connectedness, and awareness (Kieffer, 1984; Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Having appropriate or desired level of control was demonstrated to have great importance to students in their junior and senior high school environments (Boggiano & Katz, 1991; Eccles, Miller Buchanan, Flanagan, Fuligni & Yee, 1991; Hoy, 1972; Stanley, 1975) and to college students in their family environments (DeMan, 1982). Therefore, it is appropriate to consider empowerment, characterized by a sense of control, as antithetical to alienation. Normlessness, as a partial measurement of alienation, was characterized by a belief that one must engage in atypical social behaviors to achieve desired goals. Similarly, isolation was said to occur when an individual ascribed low rewards to commonly valued societal beliefs and goals. These dimensions portray a lack of agreement between an individual and the surrounding environment. Within the milieu of beliefs, varying roles, and the socialization of youth, several researchers have noted that the concept of person environment fit was important for positive outcomes (Boggiano & Katz, 1991; Eccles, .Miller Buchanan, Flanagan, Fuligni & Yee, 1991; Stanley, 1975; Hay, 1972). Aligning oneself with an activity or organization demonstrates a sense of belonging, as supported by Hendrix, Sederberg, & Miller's (1990) use of school commitment as a polar opposite to alienation. Studies have affirmed the importance of the match between students and 32 their school environments. Jones (1977) found that university students were shown to be at risk for drug use when they were experiencing loneliness or discomfort in their college environment and demonstrating vulnerability to the powerlessness dimension of alienation. This compatibility between an individual and the surrounding environment is an important consideration when measuring empowerment. Correlates of Empowerment Unlike the isolation and meaningless depicted by alienation, a sense of connectedness and purpose is part of empowerment. Studies previously discussed in this review have indicated that this empowerment was facilitated by voluntary participation in community activities (Calabrese a Schumer, 1986; Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). In one study (Calabrese & Schumer, 1986), high school students participating in a voluntary, student-controlled community service project were much lower in alienation, isolation, and discipline problems than their peers in the control group. In another example, undergraduates participating in community activities were considerably higher in political efficacy, self-efficacy, mastery, perceived competence, desire for control, and civic duty than the nonparticipants (Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Taking an active role in a community, participating in purposeful work, and following intrinsic desires appeared to have a role in discouraging the development of alienation while contributing to the advancement of empowerment for youth. Outcomes Associated with Empowerment Consistent with the notions of empowerment, several favorable outcomes have been associated with the lack of alienation at school, home, and in the community as noted in Table 2 previously. These positive outcomes can be placed in the framework of empowerment. In general, low alienation has been negatively related to longer participation in community service according to Rode (1971). Within the 33 family, alienation has been negatively correlated with positive involvement with mother and father according to Calabrese & Schumer (1986). In these instances, the lack of alienation has been associated with positive involvement with the family and community. Positive involvement and achievement at school have been often related to the lack of alienation. Less alienated students have been documented by Burbach (1972) to be much more active in extracurricular activities when alienation was measured by societal powerlessness, social powerlessness, and high school powerlessness. Students who were less alienated at school were more likely to be involved in class and varsity teams, according to Kulka, Kahle, & Klingel (1982). From these studies it can be said that school involvement is inversely related to alienation. In addition to school activities, academic achievement also appears to be negatively related to student alienation. .Although Warner & Hansen (1970) reported no differences in grade point averages among a group of alienated high school juniors, other researchers reported large differences between alienated and non-alienated youth: Burbach (1972) reported higher grade point averages among tenth to twelfth grade students scoring lower on societal powerlessness, social powerlessness, and high school powerlessness. Similarly, Hendrix, Sederberg, & Miller (1990) reported that lower alienation was related to higher grade point averages among seniors from.suburban high schools. Verma (1990) also found that high achievers were less alienated than low achievers. From these multiple sources of research, one could expect higher levels of achievement and involvement among those students less alienated. Empowered youth have been found to be more active in the community and positively involved at home and at school. They have also demonstrated higher academic achievement than alienated peers. Although these relationships are found, the direction of influence is not known. Further studies are needed to clarify the developmental nature of empowerment, involvement, and achievement in these arenas. 34 Operationalization of Measures The measurement of alienation and meaningful factors related to alienation, has been operationalized in many ways. It is important to review strategies for measurement of these important constructs. The main variables to consider are family environment, school environment, empowerment, and alienation. The family environment, as measured by the Family Environment Scale (FES), (Moos & Moos, 1981) is a useful because of its demonstrated relationship to various psychological functioning measures (See Table 8). The FES has been related to adolescent distress (Kleinman, Handal, Enos, Searight, & Ross, 1989), self-concept (Felner, Aber, Primavera, & Cauce, 1985; Nelson, 1984), adolescent achievement (Nelson, 1984), adolescent depression (Friedrich, Reams, & Jacobs, 1988), post-divorce adjustment (Farber, Felner, & Primavera, 1985), and adjustment to parental alcoholism (Dinning & Berk, 1989). Families in different situations, such as having a psychologically maladjusted child (Fox, Rotatori, Macklin, Green, & Fox, 1983), have showed differences when compared against normative FES data. Important differences have been demonstrated in families having a child in early adolescence compared to families with a child in late adolescence (Alessandri & Wozniak, 1989). Gender differences have been demonstrated in youth's perceptions of their family environment (Margalit & Eysenck, 1990). The many uses of the Family Environment Scale to assess family functioning in relation to adolescent adjustment or maladjustment suggest that it is an appropriate measure to use in assessing family functioning related to adolescent alienation. The school environment, as measured by Trickett and Moos (1974) in the Classroom Environment Scale (CES), has also demonstrated usefulness as a correlate for assessing youth adjustment (See Table 9). The CES has been related to scholastic self-concept (Felner, Aber, Primavera, & Cauce, 1985; Madonna, Bailey, & Wesley, 1990; Nelson, 1984), positive 135 nssuounn Donovnnnoo vounnIN one Geno anon» ns>nsuan consonnsoo ooonaiu ems ssso sonnet nsosno mo.v m .n~.II m man a: a: "assesssncos nesossnoo< nonueou unsosno casinos .vmon. cosnsz mo.va .ou. I n can so so unscaOOInnsn onunsnocom canusunesuno snot-nu assigns .vomn. connsz mo.va .n~.II n can so as unsoeooInnsn unansnosom nonueoo unsosnu cuoIcup .vomn. ecsnsz .mn. on nn.w nnm ov. vo.~ sensesm nauseous nnssnsn nn. I n npm as. sp.~ nuns: nounso nonncoo sno nus» on on «n lemon. nonsnnu s nnnaonu: .se 0» saw n~omn .nseuasn nauseous .momn. snom s .ucOnnssm noo.v a .On. I n mmm so an nmmn. nssnunno nsonoOnocohsm nonuaoo ono nos» on on an .soeu .nsoaa: .assensnx in: on new .~san .nsnuasn unasusnn Insane anon n .naunnasm noo.v m .mn.I I n mom a: an «own. nusnunno nsonOOnocu>sm canusnnasuno ono noon on on nn .nocm .nsoesm .emsdnsnx as: on new snnnu soanu .mman. no.v m .Nm. I n hvn s: s: canonsnnso unsunsnoo< nonueoo sauna oes cuconm season a .nsssm .connosnnm .I: on new «nan s>nnmanoz mun sonnsnuanaa annanoon Amman. non s mo.v m .mn.~ I u so we so .n> nucsosun snnesm aOnusnncsono osnonsn nucsonsnoo< .cssno .cnnxosx .nnousuom .xom as: on so. acnncsano nuasosun noocon Amman. sosso no.v m .Nn. I a omm a: an unsocooInnsn snunsnocom oncn=0nusnsm onnnsn hunUInsecn s .sns>~anna .nsn< .nsansm .nsonunno cnwn snonsn osono>no .mm. 0» m~.w no osusnsasn suasnsd. Amman. mu. I n mm s: a: >unnnuno= nonucoo sucsosun sosnnoo sns>mannm w .nsensm .noonsm asnnocoons as: an new nancsnsn an osusnsn nn scone acnn noo.v m .mn.I I n vmv s: s: nssnunno nounoOnono>nm cannsnou suesonsnoos nsnam Amman. unsm u acneeno uno annex anIan .en. on m~.w nm vo.m m~.am seasonsnoos susn no nnIun suesonsnoos mm. I n nm nn.on no.~m socsonsnoos mnnsm :Onusnncsono cun3_nsnnnssm Amman. nsnenoz s nnocsnssn< .snnsnns> saoouso. too on «9.: I I I usnsom sunm nosnnm 2 on x .snnanns> nonunosnm. nasaeonn>am >nnesm cannonsnom soasnsnsm Admmfl nuOOZ a ”002. OHMUm UGQEGOHA>GHINHHEUH find 0» OUUfldflmimchOHUUGdh HQOHWOHO£QNWQ HO nUHSOMUZ m 0.33. ns>nsuan seasonnaoo osonsIu one once unsm. ns>nsuen seasonnaoo .33st «mo ssso unnoz. 36 nssnsno no.v m .vm. I m mm as so munnnnsnOOs nssm canusnncsono s neono unsosus aennonm Anomn. nszou s unannz I sssnsno no.v m .pm. I n mm s: s: woos s>nunnom :Onus>oacn unsosus sonoonm nwomn. c0300 s uconnz no: on saw noooaoo mo.v m .nw. I n man so a: Inns» snussnocom canua>oacn unsosno cuoisup .vomn. cannsz as: on new unsoaou no.v m .m~.II n man so a: Innsn Unuusnocom nonueoo nscossa nnsosnu cuoieuh Avomn. connoz as: on new unsoeoo mo.v m .m~. I n can an s: Innsn onussnocom canusnncsono u neono onsosno cuoIcun .vmmn. connoz 3.. on 59 naoonoo 83: on. I n vm s: so Innsn ununsnocom canusnnaaono s noono unsosnu cumicuv asnnoS s .>snnsm .secoos: nonos no sosno cum osusnmaoo. Asa 0» sec nasoaoo cannessno noesosus nooeon Amman. sossu mo.v m .mn.II n omw s: a: Inns» onunsnocom cuzono ns:0nnsm onnnso >unounsoen s .sns>sannm .non< .nsensm .snos no scone cum osusnasoov As: on an. udooaoo canncsano sucsosun noocon .mmmn. sosso noo.v m .mm. I n omm we a: Inns» onunsnocom onescOnusnox ennnsd munOInsecn s .snssmfinnm .nso< .nscnsm donnnnng 28383 :8 on De 3:8 snnm uosnnm nonosnna> nonunosnm. oceanonn>cm Scannseno conuansnom soasnsnsm 2| O U) 3: .vrmn £002 a uuoxonnav onsom udoEdonnZnu Eoonnnsno can on osusnsm nonconuocsm neonmonocomnm no nonsnsoz m canon. 37 mood, and peer sociability (Wright & Cowen, 1982). This suggests that the CES is also an appropriate measure to use in assessing environmental factors in the school related to adolescent alienation. The measurement of alienation has often been based on the conceptualization of Seeman (1959) who set forth five dimensions of alienation. In his definition he proposed five dimensions of alienation. These dimensions included: 1) powerlessness - a subjective perception or objective situation in which one's behavior cannot determine outcomes; 2) meaninglessness - a "low expectancy that satisfactory predictions about future outcomes of behavior can be made (p. 786)"; 3) normlessness - a ”high expectancy that socially unapproved behaviors are required to achieve given goals (p. 788)"; 4) isolation - a situation or state that is evident when individuals ”assign low reward to goals and beliefs that are typically highly valued in a given society (p. 789.)”; and 5) self-estrangement - a situation or state in which one's behavior is determined on the basis of anticipated rewards. This foundational article was used by Dean (1961) to create the Dean.Alienation Scale used in much of the sociological literature on alienation. Dean collapsed the five concepts into three dimensions of powerlessness (.78 split—half reliability), normlessness (.73 split-half reliability), and social isolation (.84 split-half reliability). This twenty-four-item total alienation scale was found to have a .78 internal consistency reliability coefficient by Dean in his original work. Both Dean (1961) and Seeman (1959) described alienation as a non-unitary trait in which both individual and situational factors have a role. .Although widely used, Dean's alienation scale, as with many others, was developed on non-adolescent populations during another era (Mackey & .Ahlgren, 1977). Others studies have corroborated this belief (Hartley 5 iHoyy 1972; Downs, 1990). Dean actually found a normal distribution curve for the subscales of alienation on the dimensions of occupation, education, income, community type, and age. Because the hypothesis of a :negative correlation between those demographic factors and alienation 38 was rejected, and because the scores on the alienation scales extended the full range, Dean did not consider the components and scales to be artifacts. This prompted his explanation that alienation may be more of a situational variable that a personality trait. To consider the importance of settings in the origination of alienation, it was important to devise measures sensitive to alienation produced in various settings. In addition to the use of the Dean Alienation scale (1961), setting—specific measures of alienation in the school and family were devised by the author for use in the current research. These measures used the same three dimensions as understood by Dean, which include powerlessness, normlessness, and social isolation. Using these definitions of alienation it was also possible to use proxy variables, such as number of activities engaged in at school or with the family, to assess the extent of alienation in these settings. The juxtaposition of these same dimensions was used in the current research to assess empowerment. This was consistent with research by Hendrix, Sederberg, & Miller (1990) who operationalized alienation as an opposite of commitment. Reasons for the Present Study The current research examines several areas that have been inconclusive in past research. These areas to be examined include assessments of the construct validity of alienation, and the relationship of personal and ecological variables to alienation within a multivariate framework. Previous research has focused on bivariate analysis which has not clarified whether or not alienation is a predictor or outcome variable. The present research considers multiple variables within one model and examines the direction of influence to and from alienation. Examining the construct validity of alienation is one of the primary objectives of the current research. Despite theoretical discussions of the interplay of personal and environmental factors on 39 alienation, subsequent measures of alienation (often based on the work of Seeman and Dean) have primarily measured alienation as a global trait that has individual or environmental covariates (e.g., Dutt & Kureshi, 1976: Jackson, 1968; Keniston, 1965; Kulka, Kahle, & Klingel, 1982; Lanyon, 1970; Mackey & Ahlgren, 1977; and Neal & Rettig, 1967). From the empirical and theoretical foundations of knowledge two contrasting models of alienation have been offered. The theoretical model suggests that alienation is heavily linked to specific environmental settings, while the empirical model suggests that alienation is personality-based by the overwhelming use of a global measure of alienation. The disparity between the theoretical and empirical notions of this alienation construct has limited the ability of researchers and practitioners to fully understand the development of alienation. This disparity raises questions about the construct validity of alienation as a global or setting—specific measurement. Therefore, the present research focuses on major variables from.both individual and ecological domains to examine the nature of adolescent alienation. In doing so, the current research explores the usefulness of the global measure of alienation in comparison to context-specific measures of alienation for understanding the presence of alienation in adolescents. The research also compares the importance of environmental factors along with individual factors in the development of alienation in the family and in the school. These two areas of investigation will add to the present debate about the socio-psychological versus the sociological (Calabrese, 1987a) notion of alienation. Investigating the environmental factors in alienation moves this research away from the "person-centered" research that is characterized by ”person-blame" attributions (Caplan & Nelson, 1973). By including environmental and individual factors in this study, new domains of inquiry will be established and others will be replicated. This study is unique in assessing both classroom environment and family environment for their roles in facilitating or 40 hindering alienation. Other attempts will be made to replicate or clarify previous findings. This includes the previous finding that alienation decreased with age. This study will examine the impact of age on alienation with a population with an extended range of ages. The ages in youth in this study range from 12 to 19. Gender will also be included in this study to supplement previous research showing inconsistent relationships between gender and alienation. The present research is expected to clarify issues surrounding the nature of alienation and to assess individual and ecological factors in the development of alienation. Hypotheses Bivariate Hypotheses Alienation and Empowerment Alienation has been shown to be antithetical to empowerment (Rafky, 1979; Zimmerman, 1990). Additionally, students that were less alienated tended to be involved at home (Rode, 1971), at school (Burbach, 1972; Kulka, Kahle, & Klingel, 1982), and in the community (Calabrese & Schumer, 1986). Although these researchers measured alienation in general, it can be expected that alienation and empowerment are inversely related in specific settings. Therefore, the same variables that relate to alienation may also relate to empowerment. In line with previous research, the following hypotheses are stated: H: School empowerment will be negatively correlated with school alienation. H: Family empowerment will be negatively correlated with family alienation. Individual Correlates of Alienation and Empowerment Previous research has suggested that individual variables are important to consider when investigating adolescent alienation. Younger adolescents have been found to be more alienated than their older counterparts on general alienation and school alienation (Ayi-Itzhak, 41 1987; Burbach, 1972; Jackson, 1973; Jackson, 1974; Rode, 1971). Because alienation has also been linked to the home (Rode, 1971), it is also possible that younger adolescents would also be more alienated from their family than older youth. Therefore, these hypotheses are offered: H: Grade level will be negatively correlated with school alienation. H: Grade level will be positively correlated with school empowerment. H: Grade level will be negatively correlated with family alienation. H: Grade level will be positively correlated with family empowerment. Gender has also been believed to be an important consideration in youth alienation. Many researchers have included gender analyses in their inquiries, with mixed results. Both males and females have demonstrated higher levels of alienation, depending on the measures used and the age of the respondents. Although it is difficult to anticipate the response to the alienation measures used in this study, the recent work of Rogers & Gilligan (1988), has suggested that adolescent females have been at much greater risk than males to lose self-confidence. They have proposed that females receive conflicting messages about appropriate roles for women. This tension between individual desires and societal norms can be likened to Dean's concept of guidelessness. Therefore, the following hypotheses are given for further study: H: Gender will be negatively correlated with school alienation. (Females will have a higher alienation score.) H: Gender will be positively correlated with school empowerment. (Males will have a higher empowerment score.) H: Gender will be negatively correlated with family alienation. (Females will have a higher alienation score.) H: Gender will be positively correlated with family empowerment. (Males will have a higher empowerment score.) 42 Ecological Correlates of Alienation and Empowerment In addition to naming demographic variables, researchers have pursued environmental origins of youth alienation. Special attention has been paid to the family and school environments. The issue of control has important consequences for the development of health autonomy in adolescents (Steinberg, 1990). The type of control adolescents exert and have exerted over them were important contributors in the school (Calabrese, 1987b; Hartley & Roy, 1972; Hoy, 1972; Rafalides & Hay, 1971) and the home (DeMan, 1982; Rode, 1971). In both settings, research strongly suggests that authoritarian control facilitates alienation. These hypotheses are submitted in consideration of such research: H: Family control will be positively correlated with family alienation. H: Family control will be negatively correlated with family empowerment. H: Teacher control will be positively correlated with school alienation. H: Teacher control will be negatively correlated with school empowerment. In addition to appropriate levels of control, person-environment fit has been established as necessary to prevent alienation. The lack of agreement between the school setting and students' ideas about how the environment should be structured was responsible for alienating youth (Shearin, 1982; Stanley, 1975). Other studies have pointed out that students' engagement and involvement in school decreased when there was not appropriate fit between developmental needs of youth and the structure of the school setting (Boggiano & Katz, 1991; Eccles, Miller Buchanan, Flanagan, Fuligni & Yee, 1991). These studies suggest that salient features in the settings where youth spend their time should be adjusted to meet individual needs. 43 Extremes of important structural components may be unsuitable. Too much or too little of these elements are expected to foster alienation. Using the System Change/System Maintenance dimension of the environment scales (Moos & Moos, 1981; Trickett & Moos, 1974) important elements of the organizational structure can be assessed for their impact on alienation. These hypotheses suggest that the following variables facilitate student alienation when in excess or shortage. H: Family order/organization will have a "U—shaped" curvelinear relationship with family alienation to demonstrate an optimum level. Family order/organization will have an "inverted U—shape" curvelinear relationship with family empowerment to demonstrate an optimum level. Classroom order will have a ”U—shaped" curvelinear relationship with school alienation to demonstrate an optimum level. Classroom order will have an "inverted U-shape" curvelinear relationship with family empowerment to demonstrate an optimum.level. Teacher rule clarity will have a "U-shaped" curvelinear relationship with school alienation to demonstrate an optimum.level. Teacher rule clarity will have an "inverted U-shape" curvelinear relationship with family empowerment to demonstrate an optimum level. Teacher innovation will have a "U-shaped” curvelinear relationship with school alienation to demonstrate an optimum.level. Teacher innovation will have an "inverted U-shape" curvelinear relationship with family empowerment to demonstrate an optimum level. 44 Alienation Outcomes Alienation has been understood as having an isolation component (e.g., Anderson, 1973; Dean, 1961; Kolesar, 1956; Seeman, 1959; Sharma, 1988). Alienated individuals have also been seen as less engaged at home (Rode, 1971), in school work or activities (Burbach, 1972; Hendrix, Sederberg, & Miller, 1990; Kulka, Kahle, & Klingel, 1982; Verma, 1990), and in the community (Calabrese & Schumer, 1986; Zimmerman, 1990) and have achieved less academically (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Moyer & Motta, 1982). For these reasons the following hypotheses are proposed: H: Family alienation will be negatively correlated with family activities. H: School alienation will be negatively correlated with school activities. H: School alienation will be negatively correlated with school attendance. H: School alienation will be negatively correlated with school grade point average. Integrative Path Medels In addition to the hypotheses specified above, two path models were designed to encompass these individual relationships within a larger framework. Because the family and school settings are expected to have differential impacts on alienation, two separate models are hypothesized. In the family setting, several factors are believed to facilitate alienation (See Figure 1). Gender and age are expected to facilitate family empowerment. Older students and males were expected to have greater empowerment in the family. The increase in autonomy and privileges, such as driving or staying up later, is believed to increase empowerment. In keeping with previous research on gender and alienation having inconsistent findings, it is hypothesized that gender will affect alienation, although the direction is not known. As family empowerment 45 Family Alienation Family Activities 46 increases, alienation in the family declines. Family environment, including order and control, is also expected to impact family alienation. A family high in control and order may be too restrictive for youth, especially since the development of autonomy is important. The impact of family order, however, may be masked if it has the expected curvelinear relationship with family alienation. The reduced family alienation is expected to facilitated increased involvement in family activities. Youth that feel empowered in their home setting will value time spent with family. This model outlines the overall expected relationships between individual and ecological factors in the development of family alienation. In the school setting, several factors are believed to facilitate alienation (See Figure 2). Similar to the model above, gender and age are expected to facilitate school empowerment. Older students were expected to have greater empowerment at school in keeping with previous research findings. The school may also offer more autonomy and privileges to older students, such as late arrival, flexible class scheduling, senior class trips, and more leadership roles in activities. Gender is also expected to impact school alienation. .Although earlier researchers found inconsistencies in gender effects on alienation, it is hypothesized that females will be less empowered as some researchers (Calabrese & Schumer, 1986; Calabrese & Seldin, 1986; LaTorre, Yu, Fortin, & Marrache 1983) have criticized that the junior and senior high school environment is a more "masculinized" environment. As school empowerment increases, alienation in the school is presumed to decline. Order, control, rule clarity, innovation in the classroom are also expected to impact school alienation. .A classroom high in control, order, and rule clarity may be too confining for youth, especially since the development of autonomy is important. Too much innovation may also be unsettling for students. The reduced school alienation is expected to facilitate positive outcomes such as increased involvement in school activities, reduced absences, and increased grade point averages. Youth 47 School powennen CES: Rule Clarity School Alienation CES: Innovation School Activities School Absences Figure 2 48 that feel empowered at school may take greater interest in academic and extracurricular pursuits. This model outlines the overall expected relationships between individual and ecological factors in the development of school alienation. Method Sample Data was collected at a rural junior/senior high school consisting of grades seven through twelve. The total number of students enrolled at the time of the data collection was 742. Archival data (gender and grade level) was collected and analyzed for this population of 742. Grade point averages were available for the high school students (N=429) in grades nine through twelve, but not for junior high students. Because pairwise analyses were used, the total sample size for each analysis varies by the amount of valid cases available in each variable. Efforts were made to insure the maximum number of valid responses could be used. For respondents with less than 35% missing data on a given scale, the missing data was replaced with the mean. For any given analysis of variables from the questionnaire, the maximum number of respondents in the sample size was 316. Due to some remaining missing data, sample sizes as low as 311 were used in analyses involving variables contained in the questionnaire. Analyses involving the archival data utilized sample sizes ranging from 181 to 740 participants. No meaningful differences were detected between responders and non-responders on the basis of gender. A.very low correlation was found between gender (5 = .0507, conf 95% = —.02 5 g g .12) and responding to the survey. Slightly more females (2 = 163, 51.6% of respondents) than males (2 = 153, 48.4% of respondents) returned questionnaires. The total population of students in the school consisted of slightly more males (51.2%) than females (48.5%). 49 No large differences were found between responders and non- responders on the basis of grade level (See Table 10). A very low correlation was found between grade level (5 = .0168, conf 95% = -.06 g Q 5 .09) and responding to the survey. Although grade level differences were not large, a higher percentage of the respondents were from the younger grades. Low to moderate differences on grade point average and school attendance were found between students returning questionnaires and those not responding. Responders had higher grades (M = 3.02, SE = .64) than non-responders (M = 2.62, SE = .73) on a 4.0 point scale, (5 = - .2749, conf 95% = -.36 g Q 5 -.19). Non-responders also had greater days absent from.school (M = 4.05, SD = 4.36) than responders (M = 3.02, SQ - 3.79) with a resulting correlation of E = .1226, conf 95% = .05 3,9 5 .20). Findings suggest that higher achieving students and students with fewer absences are over represented in the sample of respondents. Table 10 Percentage of Respondents by Grade Level Grade Level Number of students Percentage of Total Responding 7th 64 20.3 8th 58 18.4 9th 56 17.7 10th 56 17.7 11th 48 15.2 12th 34 10.8 Design The current research employed a non-experimental cross-sectional design. The paper and pencil survey was given to students for a one- time data collection during the spring of 1993. Students were from a rural school with combined junior and senior high schools. 50 Procedures Parent permission and student assent were obtained prior to the distribution of surveys. To accomplish that task, the researcher went to the research site two weeks prior to the survey date. The teachers agreed to meet with the researcher before school that day. All teachers were given permission slip packets for their third period classes and instructions on how to distribute these materials. Each third period student was given a letter addressed to parents that explained the project and requested that parents sign and return the permission slip if they wanted their child to participate. Along with the letter was a self-addressed stamped envelope for the parents to return the permission slip. Students were told about the survey and were allowed to read the letter that was being sent to their parents. To facilitate the distribution of the permission slips to parents, these letters were mailed to parents rather than given to students to take home. Students were involved in this process because they were given large envelopes to address to their parents and enclose the permission slip and reply envelope in the larger envelope. At the end of third period classes, the researcher picked up envelopes addressed by students and mailed them from the university. Teachers reported that some students chose not to mail envelopes home to their parents. Teachers retained extra permission slip packets to distribute to students who were absent. Those students were told to take the permission slip packets home to their parents rather than having them.mailed. On the day of the data collection, the researcher returned to the third period classes to give surveys to those students who had permission slips on file. Students were read the consent information which indicated that the survey was voluntary. Some students with consent forms on file chose not to participate in the survey. Some students without permission slips on file brought permission slips with them to class and they were given surveys. Several students remarked that they had returned permission slips but they were not on file with 51 the researcher. A.list of these students names were gathered and they were given new permission slip packets to return. An unanticipated problem was later discovered with the mailing of the permission slips. A large time lag was found receiving mail from one of the small villages because mail was rerouted to a larger city before coming to the university. Many permission slips revealed a two-week delay from the date of the parents' signatures on the permission slips to the incoming envelope postmark date. Several permission slips were received in the mail after the survey collection date and efforts were made to deliver surveys to these students after the original data collection date. Teachers were given surveys 1) for their students that had permission slips on file but were absent, 2) for students that wanted to take the survey but didn't have permission slips on file, and 3) for students whose permission slips were received after the original data collection date. The researcher returned to the school two weeks later to collect surveys from.those students who were absent from school on the test day and from those students that returned pemmission slips later. Less than two dozen surveys were collected after the original data collection date. A.total of 316 students returned questionnaires, producing a response rate of 42.6% for the entire data collection. Including all permission slips received before, during, and after the data collection, a total of 387 permission slips was returned with 361 of them providing consent. Results‘ Measures and Psychometric Properties Reliability The internal consistency of the measures used in the current research are reported below as Cronbach alphas after performing a ‘In the results presented in the current research, all correlations reported or used in statistical computations have been corrected for attenuation unless otherwise specified. 52 confirmatory factor analysis of the present data. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed using multiple group analyses with communalities (Nunnally, 1978). The program that utilized this technique is called CFA, written by Hunter & Cohen (1969). Grade level of students was reported by responders on the questionnaire. Archival data was utilized to gain this information for all students, including those not completing questionnaires. This variable was assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. Age of students was reported by responders on the questionnaire and was assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. Gender of students was reported by responders on the questionnaire. Archival data was used to gain this information for non- responders. This variable was assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. Grade‘point averages (GPA) of students were obtained from archival data in school records. This information was figured cumulatively by semester starting with ninth grade unless a student transferred from another school which recorded GPA before the ninth grade. This information was assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. Student absences from school were obtained from archival data in school records and were assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. This data was recorded in whole or part days missed. School activities of students were reported on the questionnaire and were assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. Students reported which of the nine activity categories they participated in at school. Teachers noted that many activities were offered to the students in grades nine through twelve only and this may affect younger students' participation rates. Participation in family activities by students was reported on the questionnaire and was assumed to have a reliability of 1.0. Students reported which of the seven activity categories they participated in with their family. 53 A 16-item shortened version of the Classroom Environment Scale System.Maintenance System Change dimension (Trickett & Moos, 1974), was used in the self—report questionnaire. The first four items of each of the following scales in this domain were used: classroom order, rule clarity, teacher control, and innovation. Three items were dropped after performing a confirmatory factor analysis using the statistical program, CFA, written by Hunter & Cohen, (1969). The output of this analysis is contained in Appendix A. The following reliabilities (Cronbach Alphas) were found using CFA: classroom order = .74; rule clarity = .70; teacher control = .60; and teacher innovation = .65. An 8-item shortened version of the Family Environment Scale System Maintenance System Change dimension (Moos & Moos, 1981), was used in the self-report questionnaire. This domain consists of two scales, family order/organization and family control. The current research used the first four items of both scales from the standard Family Environment Scale. The organization scale was operationalized as ”the degree of importance of clear organization and structure in planning family activities and responsibilities" (Moos & Moos, 1984, page 2). The control scale was defined as "the extent to which set rules and procedures are used to run family life" (Moos & Moos, 1984, page 2). The following Cronbach Alpha reliabilities were found after performing a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA by Hunter & Cohen, 1969): organization = .59; and control = .46. The output of the confirmatory factor analysis is contained in Appendix B. Reliabilities were unable to be further improved by deleting items with low itemrtotal correlations. A.lO-item shortened version of the Dean Alienation Scale (1961) was included in the self-report questionnaire. Items were selected for their face validity and high reliability in prior pilot testing. The original Dean scale consisted of three dimensions: social isolation, powerlessness, and normiessness. Dean (1961) included helplessness, being separated from control over one's environment, and being used for others' purposes as elements of powerlessness. He conceptualized 54 normlessness as a sense of anxiety, uncertainty about goals, and being separated from group standards, perhaps as a result of great transition surrounding oneself. Social isolation was characterized by Dean as feeling estranged from group standards because of mobility and isolation. After performing a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA by Hunter & Cohen, 1969) one item was dropped and the subscales were combined into one overall alienation scale to improve the reliability of the measure. Appendix C contains the output for this analysis. This resulted in a Cronbach Alpha reliability of .71 for the total scale. Alienation Measurement Development Confirmatory factor analysis was performed using multiple group analyses with communalities (Nunnally, 1978). The program that utilized this technique is called CEA, written by Hunter & Cohen (1969). The items from the school alienation, school empowerment, family alienation, and family empowerment scales were submitted to a confirmatory factor analysis. Factor loadings confirmed the presence of these four scales (See Appendix D). School alienation & empowerment. Measures of alienation & empowerment in the school were devised by the author to measure alienation and empowerment in the specific setting of the school. These measures were included in the self-report questionnaire. Based on Dean's (1961) operationalization of alienation, a six-item.measure of School Alienation included components of powerlessness, meaninglessness, and role estrangement in the school setting. Powerlessness was an individual's perception of lacking control or ability to have participation on matters at school. Meaninglessness depicted a lack of purpose in school. Role estrangement was characterized by an individual modifying behavior to fit in at school. This six-item scale was found to have a Cronbach Alpha of .81. 55 In contrast, dimensions of powerfulness, meaningfulness, and role authenticity in the school setting were included in a six-item School Empowerment scale. Powerfulness was an individual's participation in decision-making on matters at school. Meaningfulness depicted a sense of purpose in school. Role authenticity was characterized by an individual acting in agreement with how they truly perceived themselves while at school. One of the original items from the School Empowerment scale was dropped because of its low correlation with other items in the scale, so a 5-item School Empowerment scale with a Cronbach Alpha of .72 was used in subsequent analyses. Family alienation and empowerment. Family alienation and empowerment measures were devised by the author to measure alienation and empowerment in the specific setting of the family. These measures were included in the self questionnaire. Based on Dean's (1961) Operationalization of alienation, this six- item.measure of Family Alienation includes components of Powerlessness, Meaninglessness, and Role estrangement in the home setting. Powerlessness was an individual's perception of lacking control or ability to have a say on matters at home. Meaninglessness depicts a lack of purpose at home. Role estrangement was characterized by an individual modifying behavior to fit with the family. The six—item scale was found to have a Cronbach Alpha of .79. In contrast to alienation, additional dimensions of powerfulness, meaningfulness, and role authenticity in the school setting were included in a five-item.Family Empowerment scale. Powerfulness was an individual's participation in decision-making on matters at home. Meaningfulness depicted a sense of purpose at school. Role authenticity was characterized by individuals acting in agreement with how they truly perceived themselves when they were with the family. 56 Two of the original items from the Family Empowerment scale were dropped because of their low correlation with other items in the scale, so a three-item scale with a Cronbach Alpha of .73 was used in subsequent analyses. In addition to the internal consistence measures listed above, intercorrelations of scales are provided in Appendix E. Alienation and Emppwerment Construct Validity and Measurement Quality Two models of alienation were proposed in the literature, as suggested in the earlier review in this paper. One model emphasized individual correlates of alienation, while another model stressed environmental correlates of alienation. The current research attempted to compare both models within one study by observing the correlations of alienation within and between settings. Low correlations of alienation within and among settings would support the ecological model, while high correlations of alienation within and among settings would support the personality model of alienation. Table 11 Alienation and Empowerment Correlation Matrix’ Dean Family Family School School Alienation Alienation Empowerment Alienation Empowerment Dean 1.00 Alienation Family .53 1.00 Alienation Family -.57 -.67 1.00 rment School .60 .43 -.53 1.00 Alienation School -.47 -.30 .64 -.91 1.00 Empowerment Within the ecological framework presented by various theoreticians (e.g. Bronfenbrenner, 1974; Caplan & Nelson, 1973; Dean, 1961; Seeman, 1959) the author predicted that the ecological model of alienation would ’Correlations were corrected for attenuation. 57 receive support. Because research on the nature of alienation was inconclusive, other models of alienation were also be considered. Therefore, it was important to assess the validity of the constructs designed to measure alienation in global and setting-specific situations. In doing so, one can begin to assess the strength of settings as covariates of alienation (See Table 11 above). The steps for assessing the construct validity and quality of measurement for alienation and empowerment are outlined in succeeding paragraphs. Alienation and Empowerment Quality of Measurement The first step was to examine the quality of each measure as an index of the specific type of alienation it was intended to measure. The quality of a measure is the correlation between a construct (latent variable) and the measurement variable used as an index of that variable. This correlation is equal to the square root of the reliability of the measurement of a variable. Dean's measure of alienation was found to have a standardized Cronbach Alpha reliability of .71 in the current research. Therefore, the correlation of Dean's alienation to the construct of alienation was .84. This indicated that Dean's measure of alienation was a relatively good measure of global alienation. This correlation is represented on the arrow in figure 3. The construct of family alienation was measured by a family alienation variable and a family empowerment variable. The family alienation measure was assumed to positively measure the construct of family alienation. Conversely, the family empowerment variable was expected to measure the inverse of family alienation. Family alienation was found to have a standardized coefficient alpha of .79 in the current research. The correlation of family alienation as a measure for the true construct of family alienation was .88 which indicated that the measure of family alienation employed in this research was largely able to measure the construct of alienation in the home setting. Dean Alienation School Alienation 58 School School Alienation Empowerment Figure 3 Global Alienation Family Alienation Family Alienation Family Empowerment 59 Family empowerment was found to have a standardized coefficient alpha of .73 in the current research. The correlation of family empowerment as a measure for the true construct of family alienation was -.85. This suggested that the measure of family empowerment used in this study was a relatively good measure of the opposite of alienation in the family setting. The construct of school alienation was measured positively by school alienation as a variable and negatively by school empowerment as a variable. School alienation was found to have a standardized coefficient alpha of .81 in the current research. The correlation of school alienation as a measure for the true construct was .90 which also suggested that the measure of school alienation used in this study was a relatively good measure of alienation in the school setting. School empowerment was found to have a standardized coefficient alpha of .72 in the current research. The correlation of school empowerment as a measure for the true construct of school alienation was -.85 which suggests the measure of school empowerment used in this study was a relatively good measure of the opposite of alienation at school. The measures of alienation and empowerment constructed for this study were relatively good indices of the variables they were intended to measure. The absolute values ranged from .85 to .90 for the correlations between the measurement variables and the latent traits. Construct Validity of Alienation The alienation and empowerment variables employed in this research were relatively good measures of family alienation and school alienation. Dean alienation was also a relatively good measure of the construct of general alienation. The next step in the construct validity determination was to determine the true relationship between the setting-specific constructs of family and school alienation and the construct of global alienation. 60 The relationship between family alienation and global alienation was initially unknown. This relationship, equivalent to the construct validity, was computed by dividing the original correlation of the two variables by the product of the validities of each variable. This step was analogous to correcting the correlation for attenuation. The construct validity between family alienation and Dean alienation was .53 which was moderate (Conf 95% = .45 5 Q g .61, two-sides). The relationship between school alienation and global (Dean) alienation was also unknown, and was solved as demonstrated above. The construct validity between school alienation and global (Dean) alienation was .60 which was also moderate (Conf 95% = .53 5 Q 5 .67, two-sides). Both school alienation and family alienation as constructs were relatively good measures of the construct of global alienation. An additional consideration was the relationship between school alienation and family alienation. The correlation between these measures was .43 which was moderate (Conf 90% = .36 5 Q g .51, one— sides). This finding suggests that alienation was partially generalized across these two settings. It was necessary to determine how much of this correlation was due to the shared effects of global alienation. Further analysis revealed that a correlation of .17 remained between school alienation and family alienation after the effects of global alienation were partialled out. The drop in the correlation (from .43 to .17) between family alienation and school alienation is sufficient enough to explain the commonality shared with global alienation. Therefore, much of the relationship between family alienation and school alienation is through the shared relationship with global alienation. However, there is some overlap between these two measures of alienation at home and at school since a correlation of .17 remains after global alienation has been accounted for. If the two setting- specific measures of alienation were independent of one another, there would be a correlation of zero remaining when the effects of global alienation were partialled out. Therefore, a portion of the commonality 61 in these settings was not explained by global alienation, although the partial correlation of .17 was small. Measures of alienation in each setting to not appear to be completely distinct from one another, otherwise the only relationship between school alienation and family alienation would be from the commonality shared through global alienation. (This is mathematically equivalent to .60 x .53). More commonality is found between family alienation and school alienation beyond what is shared through the effects of global alienation. The strength of the relationship between these two measures of alienation was further supported by a confirmatory factor analysis using SPSS. This analysis revealed a Cronbach alpha of .84 for the combined factor of family alienation, school alienation, and the two empowerment scales reverse coded. These considerations provided support for the strong construct validity of general alienation. One possibility for this overlap is that alienation may become generalized from one setting to another setting over time. If an adolescent feels he or she lacks purpose or meaning at home, he or she may begin to feel less positive about expectations at school. Similarly, if an adolescent's family begins making demands to alter appearance, music, or friends, the adolescent may feel an increased desire for autonomy at school. Since the school system may share some of the adolescent's parent's demands for appearance or lifestyle, the adolescent would feel additional role estrangement in the school setting. These are some possible explanations for the overlap in alienation as measured in the home and school settings. .Although there is strong support for the unitary nature of alienation, one cannot dismiss the uniqueness that does remain in the measurement of alienation in specific situations. There is a small residual of uniqueness that does appear to be due to setting variance. There may be specific elements of the home or school setting that promotes alienation that is not carried over into the other setting. An adolescent may feel incapable of having control in a school that is 62 characterized by a large student body or strict bureaucracy. Yet, this same student may feel empowered at home where he or she is part of the decision-making. Conversely, an adolescent may feel he or she has little meaning at home, but involvement in a club at school may provide a sense of purpose and meaning. Because family alienation and school alienation do not completely overlap in the measurement of alienation, setting variance remains an important consideration for the model analyzed in this paper. The present model emphasized a dynamic mixture of individual differences and the specific influence of the settings. This was assessed by measuring the relationship between latent variables and theoretical constructs. Results demonstrated moderately large correlations between Dean's global measure of alienation and the two measures of alienation in the school and family and there was a small portion of shared commonality between the two setting-specific measures of alienation that could not be explained by the shared variance with global alienation. Individual differences were moderate in explaining alienation. However, a small residual of variance remained, indicating that the home and the school can each foster alienation in ways that are unique from one another. Construct Validity of Empowerment A strong relationship was found between alienation and empowerment in the school setting, and a moderate to strong relationship was found between alienation and empowerment in the home setting (See Table 12). The correlation found between school empowerment and school alienation was -.91 (Conf 95% = -.93 5 9,5 -.89, two-sides). More distinction was found between family empowerment and family alienation. The correlation between these two measures in the home was -.67 (Conf 95% = -.73 5 Q 5 - 61, two-sides). This suggests that empowerment may be a relatively strong measure of the inverse of alienation, especially in the school setting. In I" 63 Table 12 Results of Hypotheses for Alienation and Empowerment8 Hypothesis Effect size Hypothesis Confidence interval (one—side) confirmed? Yes or No School empowerment will .2 = -.91 Yes be negatively correlated. Conf 95% = -.93 5 p 5 -.89 with school alienation Family'empowerment will .E = -.67 Yes be negatively correlated. Conf 95% = -.73 5 p -.61 with family alienation IA To consider this further, a reliability analysis was performed using SPSS to assess the reliability of both alienation scales. Family alienation included the family alienation scale and the inverse of the family empowerment scale and this Cronbach Alpha was .85. Similarly, the school alienation factor was comprised of the school alienation scale and the inverse of the school empowerment scale. This Cronbach Alpha was .86. Within the settings, alienation and empowerment were found to be highly correlated. Because these two constructs were highly correlated, it would be possible to consider these as one domain on a continuum.from alienation to empowerment rather than two distinct constructs. Empowerment and alienation within a setting appeared to have moderate validity as one construct. Across the settings, alienation and empowerment were less related. The correlation between school empowerment and family alienation was -.30 (Conf 95% = -.40 5 p 5 -.20, two-sided). .A correlation of -.53 (Conf 95% = -.61 5 p 5 -.45, two- sided) was found between family empowerment and school alienation. The correlations between alienation and empowerment were much larger within 'Correlations were corrected for attenuation. 64 the same settings than they were across different settings. This suggested there was still some uniqueness between alienation and empowerment as constructs since the correlations were less than moderate. Discriminant Validity Alienation and empowerment appeared to have moderate convergent validity as demonstrated just previously. The next consideration for validity was the extent to which school (alienation & empowerment) and family (alienation & empowerment) variables had higher correlations with similar variables but less with unrelated variables. School alienation and school empowerment were considered together because they related to other variables in an analogous fashion. Similarly, family alienation and family empowerment were examined together since they too related to variables as a combined pair. Therefore, empowerment and alienation were considered together within each setting, resulting in a school measure of alienation & empowerment and a family measure of alienation & empowerment. These two measures were considered when observing parallelism with other variables. The correlations displayed in Table 13 gave evidence to the discriminant validity of a family measure (alienation & empowerment) and a school measure (alienation & empowerment). The school measures and the family measures discriminated from one another. The school-related variables had higher correlations with the school alienation and empowerment measures than with the family alienation or empowerment measures. Grade point averages and school absences had higher absolute correlation values with school alienation and empowerment (.19 to .27) than to family alienation or empowerment (0.0 to .25). This was true of the classroom environment scales also. They were correlated much higher with school measures (.17 to .53) than to family measures (.07 to .33). The family-related variables had higher correlations with the family alienation and empowerment measures than with the school alienation or Table 13 65 Parallelism Between Alienation and Empowerment in School and Family9 School School Family Family Alienation Empowerment Alienation Empowerment Gender -0.01 0.03 0.04 0.04 Grade 0.19 -0.12 0.19 -0.13 Age 0.18 -0.12 0.17 -0.1 Grade Point 0.27 -0.24 0.19 -0.25 Average School Absences -0.19 0.24 -0.00 0.13 Family Order a 0.34 -0.48 0.67 -0.67 Organization Family Control —0.06 0.13 -0.16 0.26 Classroom Order 0.31 -0.36 0.07 -0.17 Classroom Rule 0.35 -0.53 0.12 —0.33 Clarity Classroom Control 0.17 —0.32 0.07 -0.23 Classroom 0.35 -0.51 0.11 -0.25 Innovation Dean Alienation 0.60 —0.47 0.53 -0.57 School Alienation 1.00 -0.91 0.43 -0.53 School Empowerment -0.91 1.00 -0.3 0.64 Family Alienation 0.43 -0.3 1.00 -0.67 Family Empowerment -0.53 0.64 -0.67 1.00 School Activities 0.24 -0.13 0.33 -0.25 Family Activities 0.26 -0.21 0.25 -0.36 ’Correlations were corrected for attenuation. 66 empowerment measures. The family environment scales were correlated higher with family alienation and empowerment (.16 to .67) than with school alienation or empowerment (.06 to .48). Additionally, nearly all the variables loaded in opposite direction on the empowerment and alienation measures in relatively similar magnitudes. Variables that should have correlated with related scales appeared to be relatively parallel. Variables that should not have correlated appeared to be relatively unrelated. These discriminant and parallel correlations provided further evidence for the validity of a family measure, consisting of alienation and empowerment, and a school measure, consisting of alienation and empowerment. Methodological Issues An important concern in the current research was the extent to which these results could be generalized. This was a salient issue since some students elected not to participate in the study. One potential concern was the possibility that the more alienated students dropped out of school as they became older. This issue was addressed by analyzing the drop out rate of the cohorts, the alienation scores by grade level, and the grade point averages by grade level. Potential Restriction of Range Readers may have concerns about a potential restriction of range due to differential drop out rates for older students. The alienation scores and grade point averages for each grade level (see Table 14) did not suggest that the more alienated students dropped out over time. If the most alienated students were to drop out as they grew older, the alienation score would be expected to decrease at each grade level. This would depict artificially lowered alienation scores that resulted by the omission of the alienated students scores. Should that occur, the standard deviations of the alienation scores would have been expected to shrink at each grade level as the alienated outliers dropped 67 Table 14 Alienation Outcomes by Grade Level Grade Dean Family School level Alienation .Alienation Alienation 7th grade M 3.01 3.62 3.98 M 64 64 63 §2 .61 .89 .80 8th grade M 2.96 3.49 3.88 M 58 58 57 §2 .77 .87 .89 9th grade M 2.98 3.69 4.10 M 55 56 55 §2 .68 .88 .70 10th grade M 3.08 3.74 4.21 M 56 56 56 §2 .76 .86 .62 11th grade M 3.13 4.00 4.28 M 47 47 47 §2 .69 .67 .71 12th grade M 3.17 3.94 4.24 M 34 34 33 §2 .68 .95 .82 Standard 1.06 1.09 1.09 Deviation i .16 i .17 i .17 Ratio 68 out of school. The figures in Table 14 did not reflect artificially lower alienation scores or small standard deviation scores. All three mean alienation scores increased across grade level rather than decreased, and the standard deviations were relatively similar since the standard deviation ratios were near 1.00. The standard deviation ratios reflected the ratio of the lowest grade standard deviation to the highest grade standard deviation. From this analysis, it is suggested that the scores did not reflect an omission of highly alienated drop outs. The mean alienation scores of this sample were compared to the scores of other samples to demonstrate that the alienation levels in the present were not artificially lower due to a restriction of sample. Zimmerman (1990) used the full Dean alienation scale (Dean, 1961) with a sample of 388 undergraduates, required to participate in the research as part of the class assignment, and with 205 community residents, who were recruited from voluntary organizational meetings. Zimmerman provided descriptive statistics for each group using the subscales in Dean's alienation scale. Dean's scale in Zimmerman's research utilized a 5- point likert scale analogous to the scale used in this author's research. For the undergraduate sample the mean alienation score ranged from 2.87 to 3.03 with standard deviations ranging from .50 to .58. For the community sample the range for alienation scores was 2.46 to 2.88 with standard deviations of .57 to .68. The mean overall alienation scores in the current author's work were higher than those of Zimmerman's sample. This also supported that the alienation scores in the current research were not artificially low in this non-random sample of students. An analysis of the grade point average means for each grade level also addressed concerns of a restricted range (see Table 15). If these means increased for each grade level it could have been suggested that the more alienated and less achieving students had dropped out, assuming grades were not curved. This incident would have been reflected in 69 shrinking standard deviations across grade levels. If either of these scenarios were true, it could have been argued that grade point averages were artificially inflated due to the omission of the alienated students' scores. Neither of these situations were found in the current data. The means and standard deviations of the grade point averages appeared to have a basically flat trend, suggesting that alienated dropouts were not missing from the sample. Table 15 Academic Outcomes by Grade Level Grade level GPAlo 9th grade M 2.765 M 123 §2 .813 10th grade M 2.819 M 117 §2 .692 11th grade M 2.785 M 103 §2 .726 12th grade M 2.759 M 83 §Q .630 Standard 1.286 Deviation : .131 Ratio The sample of students included in the current research did not appear to be lacking the inclusion of alienated dropouts on the basis of alienation scores or grade point averages. This suggests that the findings could be generalized. It is clear, however, that the sample of 1°7th ad 8th grade students are considered junior high students and do not have grade point averages computed by the school. 70 students included in this data collection had a higher level of academic achievement and were less likely to miss school than the school population as a whole. The students in the sample may be less alienated than their non-respondent peers, but it was not likely that a whole segment of highly alienated students were missing from this sample based on the above analysis. Additional evidence was gathered from a trend analysis of all the alienation measures across grade levels (See Table 16). The nonlinear relationships between alienation and grade levels were not characteristic of increased dropout rates for higher grade levels. The trend analysis indicated that the linear components were larger than the nonlinear components. Because the relationships were primarily linear, there was no evidence of any drops in the alienation scores which would have been evident if a group of alienated students were missing from the sample due to dropping out of high school in the later years. Table 16 Trend Analysis of Alienation Types as a Function of Grade Level Correlation Trend Analysis Linear Non- Linear Nonlinear Eta 95% Alienation linear Confidence Type (two-sided) School E = eta = 511) = 9.39 F(4) = .99 .07 5 n 5 .27 .17 .20 pp = .00 p = .45 Family 5 = eta = F(l) =10.85 F(4) = .39 .06 5 n 5 .26 .18 .20 p = .00 p = .82 Dean 5 = eta = F(l) = 2.84 F(4) = .39 .00 5 n 5 .16 .10 .12 p = .09 p = .82 (Probability that eta is zero is 0.077) 71 Bivariate Hypotheses Individual Correlates Gender and grade level were expected to contribute to alienation. Table 17 describes the outcomes for both grade level and gender hypotheses in more detail. Grade level was hypothesized to be negatively correlated with both school alienation and family alienation and positively correlated with measures of empowerment in the school and family. None of these hypotheses were supported (see Table 17). The resulting correlations were opposite of the predicted direction. In each case, however, the absolute values of the correlations were less than .20 suggesting that the relationships between grade and both alienation or empowerment were not strong. These findings were contrary to the studies reviewed earlier in this paper that suggested younger students are more alienated and less empowered. The author expected to replicate the findings of Burbach (1972) who measured alienation in a sample of rural high school students similar to the sample employed in the current research. In that study, a small relationship was found to suggest that older students were more alienated and less empowered than their younger counterparts. Gender was also an individual variable expected to be correlated with measures of alienation and empowerment. At home and at school females were expected to be more alienated than males and males were expected to be more empowered. The findings did not strongly support these hypotheses. Resulting correlations were in the predicted direction in three out of the four hypotheses (see Table 17 above) but all correlations were small. Results indicated that males in the present sample were more empowered at home and at school as expected. As hypothesized, females were found to be somewhat more alienated at school than males. The remaining hypothesis was in the opposite of the predicted direction. Unexpectedly, males were more alienated in their families than were females. Due to the magnitude of these correlations and the presence of zero in all four confidence intervals, no 72 Table 17 Results of Hypotheses of Individual Correlates Hypothesis Grade level will be negatively Effect size and 95% Confidence Interval (one-sided) r = .19 Hypothesis confirmed? No: wrong correlated with school alienation .1 < p < .28 direction Grade level will be negatively E = .19 No: wrong correlated with family alienation .10 < p < .27 direction Grade level will be positively E = -.12 No: wrong correlated with school empowerment —.21 < p < -.03 direction Grade level will be positively E = -.13 No: wrong correlated with family empowerment -.22 < p -.04 direction Females will be more alienated E = -.01 No: 57% from school than males (negative -.10 5 p _ .08 chance of correlation) negative correlation Females will be more alienated E = .04 No: 75% from the family than males -.05 5 p .13 chance of (negative correlation) positive correlation Males will be more empowered at E = .03 No: 69% school than females (positive -.06 5 p .13 chance of correlation) positive correlation Males will be more empowered in E - .04 No: 75% the family than females (positive -.05 5 p _ .13 change of correlation) positive correlation 73 substantial relationships were demonstrated between gender and both alienation or empowerment in either setting. After considering individual covariates of alienation, namely grade level and gender, it was apparent that neither variable was largely related to alienation nor empowerment at home nor at school. Attention was next given to the possible role for ecological variables in alienation and empowerment Ecological Correlates of Alienation and Empowerment Ecological covariates, in addition to individual variables, were also assessed for a potential relationship with alienation and empowerment. Dimensions of students' family and school environments were assessed for their linear and nonlinear relationship to alienation and empowerment in specific settings. In these cases, curvelinear relationships were predicted. The graphical representations of these relationships were specified as ”U-shaped" or inverted "U-shaped.” These hypotheses were tested by using the REGRESS program (Hunter 5 Cohen, 1969). Initially these regression equations were set up to include these orthogonal components: x = independent variable, (x-M)2== (independent variable - independent variable mean) squared, and y = dependent variable. Entering these components into the REGRESS program yielded b weights. The linear b weight (b,), nonlinear b weight (b,), and constant (bo)‘were used to solve for the "a" coefficients. The coefficients were solved as follows: a0 = bo + b,x2, a1 = b1 - 2b2X, a2 = bL These coefficients were translated into the regression equation Y = a0 + alx + a,X2. Classroom environment. Several facets of the school environment were expected to facilitate alienation when present in diminished or excessive quantities. This relationship would be represented by a "U-shaped" curvelinear relationship. This would mean that alienation would be 74 greatest when order, rule clarity, or innovation were too great or too little. In a related fashion, empowerment was expected to be achieved when these elements of the school environment were present in moderate amounts. This would be represented by an "inverted U-shape" curvelinear relationship, suggesting that empowerment would be highest when order, rule clarity, and innovation were found in stable quantities rather than extremes. Results of these hypotheses are displayed in Table 18. All classroom environment characteristics were found to have linear relationships with school alienation and school empowerment, so none of the curvelinear hypotheses were supported. In all regression equations the linear b weights were larger than the nonlinear b weights. However, the nonlinear components that were present in each regression equation were consistent with the predicted direction. The classroom environment dimensions had positive linear relationships with school alienation and negative linear relationships with school empowerment. The following linear relationships were found where curvelinear relationships were expected: 1) As classroom order increased, school alienation increased and school empowerment decreased. 2) As teacher rule clarity increased, school alienation increased and school empowerment decreased. 3) As teacher innovation increased, school alienation increased and school empowerment decreased. The magnitudes of the relationships between the variables in the regression equation were large, as evidenced by the Multiple R values in Table 18, for teacher rule clarity and teacher innovation when compared to both school alienation and empowerment, and for classroom order when compared with school empowerment. The relationship was moderate for classroom.with school order. The classroom environment variables had consistent relationships with school alienation and empowerment and the relationships were moderate to large, although the relationships were primarily linear, rather than non- linear. These regression analyses provide inconclusive information for assessing the impact of these classroom characteristics on school alienation and school empowerment. '75 .nssnounn cocoonncoo one pennnIOQOS .2335 85328 .3 3.3.5.5.. .osonsiozu sns una>nsuen consonneoo nns .osuoeso ssnznscuo snsnnaz .vN.v ou mo.~. SAMN. 0» ho.Iv .vn.I ou om.I. neeanszooss noocon can: oneneOnusnsn nssann as» mm. nssenn no.m so. ~n.I s>nusose e s>sc nnn3.nonneoo nscossa noconun canuscsnns you. n~p.n 0» cm.~. anvn. on on.I. .nv. on no.. noocos can: oncscOnusnsn nssnnn no» on. nssenn no.n No.I nw. ssnunnoo s s>sc nnnz nanueoo nscossa uassnssooas noocos .mm.v on mv.mv .o~. on oo.I. awn.I ou om.I. cun3_oncnc0nusnsn nssennssnso IooscnI: oz mm. nssenn va.n mo. ov.I osnns>sns ea s>sc nnn3 eOnus>ocen nscossa acnusesnns .nn.n ou mo.~. zen. 0» an.Iv .mm. on n~.. noocou can: oncneOnusnsn nascnns>nso oz on. nssenn oo.~ mo.I an. InsoscnIDI s s>sc nnn3 canus>oeen nseossa unsanszooas noocos can: oneneOnusnsn .pn.p on an... zoo. 0» ~m.Ic .sm.I on hn.nI. nascnns>nso IsoscnI: osuns>enu oz om. nssenn or.m m~.I oh.I as s>sc nnn3.>unnsno snsn nsrossa noconun canusesnns noocon non. .mp.n on mo.. .vw. on on.I. .vh. on oo.. cun3.oncn:0nusnsn nssenns>nso Iososcn oz mm. nssenn vm.~ no.I en. ID: a s>sc nnvz >unnsno snsn nscossa ueosnssooas noocos .no.n ou mm.~. .on. on mo.I. non.I on ~v.I. can: oncnnOnusnsn nssenns>nso Iooscn oz hm. nssenn m~.n mo. on.I I: usuns>enc as s>ss nnn3.nsono soonnnsno aOnusesnns .mn.n cu vm.~v Ano. on nN.I. nov. 0» an.. noocon can: oneseOnnsnsn nssenns>nso oz vm. nssenn no.m mo.I an. IososcnIDI s s>sc nnn3.nsono soonnnsno .osonnusco AnsonnIseo AnsonsIseo .soasonncoo ammo .socsonncoo omm. .soesonneou ammo «osEnnneoo m nsscnneoc ozone: n prone: o nnnszuon>x snonunsz no nascnn unsuscoo nascnneoz nssenn nnnscuoohz sususnsnnoo nounoOnoou noocom no senscnomNmINunnssenn no nunsnsm an Onnmh 76 For other dimensions of the classroom environment, linear relationships were expected. Based on previous studies outlining the strong role of control in student alienation, teacher control was hypothesized to be positively correlated with school alienation and negatively correlated with school empowerment. As shown previously in Table 18, teacher control was shown to have a strong linear relationship to both school alienation and empowerment. This was explored further through correlational analyses. Both hypotheses were supported. Students who perceived their classrooms to have higher levels of teacher control had higher levels of school alienation and lower levels of empowerment. This suggested that elevated levels of teacher control could have negative impacts on student psychological outcomes. The results of these hypotheses are detailed in Table 19. Table 19 Results of Correlational Hypotheses of School Ecological Correlates Hypothesis Effect size Hypothesis 95% Confidence confirmed? Interval (one- Yes or No sided) Teacher control will be positively E = .17 Yes: but correlated with school alienation .08 <53 < .26 small effect -.32 Yes Teacher control will be negatively E correlated with school empowerment .40 < 2.< -.23 Family environment. For family environment variables, both linear and curvelinear relationships were predicted for alienation and empowerment. The order or organization of the family environment was expected to have a "U- shaped” curvelinear relationship to family alienation. Additionally, family empowerment was expected to have an ”inverted U-shape" curvelinear relationship to family order/organization. It was expected 77 that too much or too little family order or organization would have undesirable outcomes. A moderate amount of family organization was believed to provide optimum outcomes. Family organization had strong linear relationships with family alienation and empowerment, as evidenced by the large Multiple R values rather than curvelinear relationships. The direction of these relationships, however, were positive and negative as predicted. Higher levels of family organization were associated with increased alienation and decreased empowerment in the family. This suggests that family order and organization has positive effects on the psychological outcomes for children. These hypotheses were not supported, although the direction of the relationships were predicted correctly. Other family variables were expected to have linear relationships. Family control was anticipated to have a linear association with family alienation and empowerment. Increased family control was believed to be related to increased alienation and decreased empowerment at home. Neither hypothesis was fully supported. .Although family control was related to family alienation in a strong linear fashion indicated by the Multiple R value, the relationship was negative rather than positive. As family control decreased, so did alienation in the home. Family control was found to have a strong non-linear relationship to family empowerment, however the relationship was positive rather than negative. A.moderate level of family control was associated with family empowerment outcomes, whereas extreme amounts were associated with family alienation outcomes. These results are displayed in greater detail in Table 20. Because family control was originally hypothesized to have a linear relationship to family alienation and empowerment, correlational analyses were conducted. It was expected that family control would be positively correlated with family alienation and negatively correlated .2533 85328 .2 332.8: 42633 8.3328 .2 SunnIonoa .osonnIozu sns nnu>nsuan consonneoo nns .osuoeso snnznscuo «none: .omo one uns>nsuen seasonneoo nnt! '78 unsansmommmlxmmmmmlI .84 on Si .13. on on; .8. on mo; 53 358328 nascnn 2.332. oz 2.. 5057.82 3. no. 3. a 25: nnn: nonnnoo Sean canusesnnn znnamn 83038 and on 3.: .12... on 8.; .3... on Si. 5? angnaonnansn name: 2.338 neon) “oz nv. nsocnn mv.m mm.I on.I a s>sc nnnz nonueoo >nnesm nessnssommm unnasn can: oneseOnusnsn nssennssnso 8mm 3 8.: .8. on SI. SCI 3 run; 392?: usnnssan. 5 32 oz S. 323 «mm on: at nnnz nonnannamunotsuno Sean :Owumccndo been 5? 3:28:23 and on 3.. .3. on so.-. .84 on 3.. naoanngnao Icon—2?? a 2.2 oz mm. nssenn mv.n on. he. nnnz acnusnnesono\nsono annesm Gonna =83» Gonna oz no no» I03» .socoonneoo ammo I03» .socsonneoo .mm. I039 .soesonncoo omm. «ossnnneoo z nssenneoz masons: o aprons: a unnscuoonz snonunsz no nsscnn ueauneoo nsssnneoz nsocnn nnnscuoo>m :nsusnsnnoo noun snoom nnasm no assoc» z nunnssann no nunsnem ON Innwa 79 with family empowerment. Neither hypothesis was supported since the resulting correlations were opposite of the predicted direction. Consistent with the findings in the regression analysis, increased family control was associated with decreased alienation and increased empowerment in the home. Table 21 contains further details of these correlational analyses. Table 21 Results of Correlational Hypotheses of Family Ecological Correlates Hypothesis Effect size 95% Hypothesis Confidence confirmed? (One—sided) Yes or No Famdly control will be positively £.= -.16 No: wrong correlated with family alienation -.25 < p < -.07 direction Family control will be negatively r = .26 No: wrong correlated with family empowerment .18 < p < .35 direction Alienation Outcomes Alienation in the home and at school was presumed to have consequences concerning family involvement, school involvement, and academic progress. These findings are explained further in Table 22. Alienation was expected to have negative relationships to participation in family activities, participation in school activities, and grade point averages. .Alienation was predicted to have a positive relationship with school absences. Results from the correlational analyses revealed relationships opposite to those predicted. The magnitude of the correlations are less than moderate. These results were found: 1) Family alienation increased with participation in family activities. 2) School alienation increased with participation in school activities. 3) Academic achievement as measured by grade point averages increased with school alienation. 4) School absences decreased with school alienation. These results were unexpected. From.these analyses I 80 it would appear that the alienated student was more involved with activities at home and at school, had higher grade point averages and had fewer school absences. Table 22 Correlation Results of Alienation Outcomes in Family and School Hypothesis Effect size Hypothesis 95% Confidence confirmed? (One-sided) Yes or No Famdly alienation will be negatively 5 = .25 No: wrong correlated with family activities .16 < p < .34 direction School alienation will be negatively E = .24 No: wrong correlated with school activities .1 < p < .33 direction School alienation will be positively 5 = -.19 No: wrong correlated with school absences —.28 < p < -.10 direction School alienation will be negatively E = .27 No: wrong correlated with grade point average .18 < p < .38 direction Integrative Path Models Path models were constructed to delineate the relationships of individual and ecological variables to alienation and empowerment as outlined in previous figures 1 and 2. A model was created for each setting of family and school. Because it was not known prior to the study that alienation overlapped in each setting, two independent models were designed. Because confirmatory factory analysis and correlational analyses determined that alienation and empowerment were strongly related, the empowerment scales were inversely coded and combined with the alienation scales in each setting. This resulted in a family alienation scale comprised of family alienation and empowerment items and a school alienation scale containing the school alienation and empowerment items. The hypothesized path models were analyzed using the PATH program.written by Hunter & Cohen (1969) which resulted in path coefficients. The software also analyzed the indirect links as well. 81 The final models demonstrated an overall goodness of fit with the data and no weak links were present to suggest that the model deviated from the data. Family Environment The original family environment path model showed a strong degree of fit and required no additional modifications (g! = 2.68 (4), p=.612) (See Figure 4). The individual variables of gender (B = .08; 68% confidence .00 S B 5 .15) and age (B = .05; 68% confidence -.03 S B 5 .12) did not conclusively predict alienation in the family. Ecological variables were more useful in predicting family alienation. Family order and organization had a strong relationship (B = .78; 68% confidence .73 S B 5 .83) with family alienation, suggesting that increased family order facilitated alienation from the family. .A moderate to large relationship (B = -.37; 68% confidence -.45 g B 5 - .29) showed that a decrease in family control predicted higher levels of alienation. Surprisingly, increased alienation moderately predicted an increased level of involvement in family activities (B = .33; 68% confidence .26 5 B 5 .40). One possibility was that students feeling alienated from their families attempted to mitigate these feelings by becoming more involved in family activities. While gender and age did not seem to have a role in fostering family alienation, higher levels of family order and lower levels of family control were moderate to strong in predicting the alienation outcome which then lead to increased involvement with family activities. An analysis of the indirect links revealed no need for modifications. Tests of specific parts of the model showed that no parts of the model lacked fit. Table 23 reveals only small errors for the reproduced correlations subtracted from the actual correlations, and none of these errors were significant. This proposed model of family alienation showed a high level of fit in its original design, encompassing individual and ecological factors. Age Figure 4 .05 i .07 78i .05 82 .08 i .08 -.37 i .08 Family Alienation .33 i .07 Family Activities 83 Table 23 Correlations for Family Alienation Path Model‘7 Reproduced correlations: Errors: (Actual - reproduced) 2 4 7 8 19 21 2 4 7 8 19 21 2 100 -5 -6 8 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 -9 4 -5 100 16 2 18 6 4 0 0 0 0 0 14 7 -6 16 100 17 77 28 7 0 0 0 O 0 -5 8 8 2 17 100 —19 -7 8 0 0 0 0 0 -1 19 0 18 77 -19 100 36 19 0 0 0 0 0 0 21 0 6 28 -7 36 100 21 -9 14 -5 -1 0 0 2 = Gender 4 = Age 7 = Family Order 8 = Family Control 19 = Family Alienation 21 = Family Activities School Environment The path model for the school environment (See Figure 5) showed a moderate level of fit in its final version and required only one additional modification (g3 = 23.67 (20), p = .257). The only deviation from the original model was the deletion of the grade point average variable. This variable did not increase the ability of the model to explain the data so it was removed. Similar to the family alienation model above, the individual variable path coefficients of gender (B = -.03; 68% confidence -.13 5 B 5 .07) and age (B = .14; 68% confidence .04 5 B 5 .24) were not able to predict alienation in the school setting with a high level of certainty. Ecological variables that characterized the school environment were able to predict school alienation better than the individual variables. Just as in the family model above, ecological variables contributed more to the model than the individual variables. 17Two decimal positions are implied in the matrices. .14 i .14 ® 6i.11 84 32i.ll CES: School Alienation CES: Innovation School Activities 85 The model revealed that increased classroom innovation, rule clarity, and order moderately predicted higher levels of student alienation at school. Classroom innovation moderately predicted school alienation (B = .32; 68% confidence .21 5 B 5 .43). Teacher rule clarity (B = .19; 68% confidence .6 5 B 5 .31) and classroom order (B = .16; 68% confidence .05 5 B 5 .27) were less strong in their predictions of school alienation. Classroom control did not predict school alienation with certainty (B = .02; 68% confidence -.10 5 B 5 .13). Consequences of alienation were small to moderate in this model. School absences were shown to moderately decrease with increased alienation (B = —.22; 68% confidence -.31 5 B 5 -.13). Involvement in school activities was found to increase with increases in school alienation (B = .19; 68% confidence .10 5 B 5 .28). .Although the magnitudes of these predictions were meaningful, both were in found to be opposite of the predicted direction. One possibility was that these students who missed fewer days were more involved in school activities were actually more alienated from school because the quality of education was not meeting their expectations. They may have compensated for their disappointment by taking advantage of activities at school. These surprising results warrant further study. School alienation was originally hypothesized to directly impact grade point average. The overall chi square value remained the same with or without the direct link from.alienation to grade point average, therefore the link was dropped because it did not increase the explanation of the data. An analysis of the indirect links revealed no need for modifications. Tests of specific parts of the model showed that no parts of the model lacked fit. Table 24 reveals only small errors and none of these errors were significant. This model of school alienation had an overall good fit with the data with only one minor revision to its original design. It revealed that ecological rather than individual factors were better able to predict alienation at school. 86 Table 24 Correlations for School Alienation Path Model18 Reproduced correlations: 2 4 9 10 11 12 18 20 6 5 2 100 -5 1 5 -1 2 -2 0 0 0 4 -5 100 24 2 3 -8 16 3 -4 2 9 1 24 100 42 45 22 35 7 -8 4 10 5 2 42 100 48 55 44 8 -10 5 11 -1 3 45 48 100 15 23 4 -5 2 12 2 -8 22 55 15 100 45 9 -10 5 18 -2 16 35 44 23 45 100 19 -22 11 20 O 3 7 8 4 9 19 100 -4 20 6 0 -4 -8 -10 -5 -10 -22 -4 100 -32 5 0 2 4 5 2 5 11 20 -32 100 Errors: (Actual - reproduced) 2 4 9 10 11 12 18 20 6 5 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 -18 6 -14 4 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 11 11 -25 9 0 0 0 0 0 O 0 -17 5 -4 10 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 -12 -1 -16 11 O 0 0 0 0 0 O -6 4 2 12 0 0 O 0 0 0 0 -12 -8 -9 18 0 0 0 0 0 0 69 0 0 14 20 -18 11 -17 -12 -6 -12 0 116 1 22 6 6 11 5 -1 4 -8 0 l 35 -11 5 —14 -25 -4 -16 2 -9 14 22 -11 43 2 = Gender 4 - Age 9 = Classroom Order 10 = Classroom Rule Clarity 11 = Classroom Control 12 8 Classroom Innovation 18 = School Alienation 20 - School Activities 6 = School Absences 5 = Grade Point Average 1"Two decimal positions are implied in the matrices. 87 Discussion The present study contributed to the theoretical and measurement issues related to alienation. It furthered understanding of the nature and structure of alienation (See figure 3 earlier) and provided valid instruments for the assessment of alienation and empowerment in both the home and the school. These findings contributed to theories of alienation: (1) The data supported a unitary construct of alienation because alienation in the family and school settings were strongly related to the global measure of alienation. (2) Alienation and empowerment were strongly related and could be considered on opposite ends of one continuum. (3) Age was unrelated to alienation in the family, but alienation at school increased with age, although the relationship was small. (4) Gender was not related to alienation or empowerment in either setting. (5) Overall, ecological variables were stronger than individual variables in predicting alienation and empowerment within both settings. (6) School alienation was increased by elevations in classroom innovation, teacher control, rule clarity, and order. (7) Increased family order was strongly related to family alienation and decreased family control was moderately related to increased family alienation. (8) The relationships between environmental characteristics and measures of alienation and empowerment were primarily linear, rather than curvelinear. (9) Increased alienation predicted increased involvement in family and school activities, along with fewer absences from school and higher grade point averages. Because many of these findings were unexpected further investigation of these relationships is necessary. Nature of Alienation As foundational researchers in the field of alienation, Dean (1961) and Seeman (1959) proposed that alienation was a non-unitary trait. They stated that individual and situational factors had a role in fostering alienation. Although this interactionist perspective has 88 been taken by many (e.g., Fromm, 1964: Merton, 1968; Wynne, 1978), alienation has been primarily measured as a global personality trait without regard to situations. Apart from the few researchers who have used alienation measures specific to the school setting (e.g., Burbach, 1972, Kolesar, 1956; Kulka, Kahle, & Klingel, 1982; Rafky, 1979), alienation has been measured as an individual trait. This overwhelming disparity between theoretical perspectives (which include setting-specific variables) and measurement practices (primarily individual difference variables) has fostered a dilemma. It has not been possible to discuss whether alienation is largely a setting-specific or comprehensive phenomenon. As early as 1965, Dean called for construct validity studies to assess the dynamic nature of alienation. This task has been neglected for many years. The present research attempted to integrate both perspectives. This interactional approach included both setting-specific measures and individual difference measures. Earlier in this paper, a review of literature suggested that the school, home, and community have been important environmental determinants of alienation. Previous research noted that alienation has been associated with age and partially with gender. The school and family settings were also expected to be important contributors to alienation. This thinking is consistent with Bronfenbrenner's (1977) ecological theory of human development that stresses the interaction of multiple systems. The present study examined the interaction between individual and systemic factors in alienation. Moderate support was found for a general unitary construct of alienation, although there were still unique elements of alienation in each setting. School alienation and family alienation accounted for a moderate portion of the general alienation measured, although not entirely. Several foundations for these findings were explored. The commonality of alienation in both settings was quite high. This was supportive of the view of alienation as a personality trait. 89 This finding may be partially accounted for by theories of personality. Common to all personality theories (Maddi, 1972), are the beliefs in core tendencies and traits altered through developmental processes and become routinized in dealing with the surrounding environment. On this basis, alienated adolescents would be expected to consistently estrange themselves from their settings, and a general dissatisfaction would be carried over into other areas of life. Following the personality theories, certain individuals would be continually prone to alienation throughout their lifespan or across a variety of settings. The moderate correlation between general societal alienation and localized environments may be attributed to inherent traits or dispositions. Alienation as a person-specific phenomenon may also be a result of time-specific factors related to adolescence. .Alienation may be characteristic of the ego-diffusion state delineated by Erikson (1950). In this state of ego-diffusion, youth have not developed a clear picture of the world at large or their relationship to it. In this framework, ego-diffusion is a normal state that will be overcome with age as identity formation occurs. Similarly, Fromm's (1946) framework also suggests that alienation will decrease when individuation and self- realization occur. This process is said to occur as youth grow stronger physically, emotionally, mentally, and break ties with the family. In both these instances, alienation might be expected to dissipate following the normal period of self-discovery in adolescence. Alternate explanations can be found within an ecological framework. The unitary nature of alienation may be indicative that many settings are alienating. Bronfenbrenner (1974) observed that the primary institutions of the United States were failing. He credited these evolutionary changes in settings, such as the schools, courts, clinics, prisons, workplace, and family, as being responsible for alienating young people from adults. Since adults are the primary shareholders of the family, school, and society, it would not be 90 surprising to find that adolescents are alienated from both places and from society in general. Such alienation from institutions may be a result of deteriorating institutions, or this may reflect a normal developmental process. Coleman (1961) concluded that involvement with peers facilitated socialization into a peer culture. This preoccupation was believed to alienate adolescents from adult society. This explanation suggests that unitary alienation may be a transitory state. It is based on the belief that adolescents are being simultaneously alienated from adult institutions, rather than possessing a personality trait that tends toward disengagement. Neither explanation alone sufficiently illustrates the relationships demonstrated in figure 3 earlier in this paper. An interactionist approach offered by Kurt Lewin (1935) that acknowledges individual and ecological factors are best suited to this data. This theory suggests that a person's self-concept or personality may predispose an individual to alienation. Additionally, environmental forces serve to strengthen or inhibit the individual self-concept. Lewin suggests that this is particularly likely to occur in adolescence when individuals are between child and adult roles and have no clear membership in either group. The Lewinian theory of behavior as a function of person and environment best explains'the nature of alienation found in the present study. Nature of Empowerment The present study also contributed to a further understanding of the nature of empowerment. Empowerment was found to be highly correlated with alienation in a completely inverse relationship in both the family and school settings. From this it was evident that this measure of empowerment was at the opposite end of a continuum between empowerment and alienation. This finding adds to the research seeking to understand the components of empowerment for youth (Zimmerman, 1990) as well as adults (e.g., Zimmerman, Israel, Schulz, & Checkoway, 1992). 91 Individual Variables .Although the findings of a unitary nature of alienation and empowerment discussed above suggest that ecological factors are not distinctive in the overall measurement of alienation, the present study found ecological variables to be more important than individual variables in predicting alienation and empowerment within the different settings. Demographic variables of age and gender were generally not supported as principle contributors to alienation. Gender was virtually unrelated to both alienation and empowerment in each setting of home and school. Grade level was less than moderate in predicting these same outcomes. Although not profoundly, older students were generally found to be more alienated and less empowered than their younger counterparts. This contradicts previous research from.previous decades. One explanation is that in the present society, more pressures than freedoms accompany maturation. Uncertain job prospects and a changing society may make decisions about work or higher education more difficult and more important. Perhaps the educational system is not responding to students concerns about the future since older students were less empowered at school than the younger students who may not be facing these complex decisions. It is not clear, however, that age is a salient component of alienation. Ecological Variables & Outcomes of Alienation Ecological variables were more associated with alienation than were the individual variables. The regression analyses indicated that the characteristics of the family and home were related to alienation and empowerment in linear, rather than nonlinear, ways. The direction of these relationships were generally unexpected. For the classroom, increased innovation at school was the strongest predictor of increased alienation. This was closely followed by reduced levels of teacher control as a predictor of school alienation. Perhaps the most alienated students were in classrooms 92 where teachers were giving students more control in the classroom.and employing non—traditional methods and to engage the students in learning and having a positive experience at school. This may be reflected in the finding that the more alienated students are more likely to have higher grade point averages and fewer days absent from school. Questions still remain in relationship to this finding. It is unclear why these students with higher levels of alienation would be doing better in school, perhaps as a result of more control and innovation in the classroom. This is particularly puzzling since items in the alienation scale include feelings of not having control and having other people running ones life. One direction to consider is that the profile of the alienated student may actually include the high achieving student who perceives that the educational system is failing. These students may still excel in school and take advantage of school activities, but still feel that the school system is not meeting their expectations. Further research is needed to create a better describe who the alienated students are and what contributes to this paradoxical finding. In the home increased family order and organization was very strong in predicting family alienation. Decreased levels of family control were moderate in predicting family alienation. The present findings were incongruous with previous research. Other studies clearly outlined the role of increased control as a predictor of alienation. For this sample, it appears that highly ordered families, rather than highly controlling families, produced alienating situations for the children. From this data, the profile of the alienating family appears to be one in which daily life is very routinized and carefully planned but there is little guidance given to the children. It is conceivable that the members of these families are very busy with little time to invest in the emotional ties with children. This could also explain why the more alienated students are more involved in family activities. Children from these families may take advantage of opportunities to 93 spend time with the family to increase the connections to the family that may be lacking in the ordinary routine. It would be important to further investigate children's perceptions of their family life and how that may contribute to a sense of psychological alienation. Summary moderate relationships were found among the various measures of alienation, suggesting that alienation consists of individual and environmental factors, although environmental factors were more pronounced. Empowerment was also found to be highly contrasted with alienation in such a way that it may be considered a polar opposite on one continuum. The hypothesized personality and environment variables were differential in predicting alienation. Older students were more alienated than younger students in both settings. Gender was unrelated to alienation. Both individual and ecological factors were considered in two path models that appeared to fit the data. In these models family and school characteristics contributed to alienation. This setting-specific alienation was able to moderately predict academic outcomes and activity levels at home and school. Several findings were inconsistent with expectations and further investigation is warranted to understand person-environment fit and students' perceptions of their environments. Recommendations Recommendations for action and future research are offered based on the present research. This initial study raises questions about the dual role of personality and environments in the development of alienation. Because alienation is related to both individual and environmental factors, additional research is needed. The present study also offers a framework for alienation interventions. 94 Research efforts to clarify some of these issues should center on several issues. (1) Additional methods of assessments should be employed. In-depth assessments of students and their environments are necessary to gain a better profile of the alienated students, their families and their peers. Case studies, interviews, and field observations could be useful to gain a broader perspective of the alienated students. (2) Further research should also ask students about what their desired environments are in addition to describing the actual environment. Perhaps alienation is more related to the divergence of ideal versus actual environments, rather than on specific environmental characteristics alone. (3) It would be useful to identify the additional environments that are important in the lives of these youth and assess whether students are alienated from these settings as well. This would help to answer whether alienation is still a unitary construct when is it measured in settings outside adult-oriented environments such as the school and home. (4) Additional samples should be assessed to determine if contextual factors unique to the current sample help to explain the findings that were inconsistent with previous research. Variables such as rural vs. urban, local economy type, population size and population composition could help explain differential meanings of alienation, family characteristics, and school characteristics. Communities have differing expectations for youth and their behaviors that may contribute to differences in alienation findings with this sample. To further explain this notion of global alienation, it would be useful for researchers to observe levels of alienation in non-adult settings. If adolescents are estranged from institutions governed by adults, as a result of peer-group intensification or by simply being disregarded by adults, than we may find that adolescents can be alienated from school while simultaneously being empowered in peer group 95 settings. Further research into youth oriented or non-adult dominated settings may provide additional answers to the question regarding globalized alienation. This question can also be addressed by employing a longitudinal design to assess alienation over time. If alienation is more characteristic of an individual personality, we would expect that to remain stable over time. If, on the other hand, alienation diminishes as youth enter meaningful adult roles, alienation can be attributed to the context of adolescence. As suggested by developmentalists, alienation may be specific to the period of adolescence due to ego diffusion and self-realization. Further longitudinal research is necessary to compare theories of alienation as a state versus a trait, and to consider the role of adolescence as a time-specific variable. Findings from this research can impact interventions designed to ameliorate alienation. The present study suggests that increasing empowerment is akin to alleviating alienation. Using the dimensions of empowerment measured in this study, community workers should increase the sense of powerfulness, meaningfulness, and role authenticity experienced by the youth. Within the framework of Kurt Lewin, the environment moderates the personal concept. Therefore, these components should be addressed on an individual level and an environmental level. Further intervention efforts based on the present study should seek to give adolescents a more salient role as decision-maker in the family, school, community, and society to reduce alienation. From a review of studies to date, the author suggests that a reduction in alienation would be expected when the following conditions occur: 1) Youth increase their ability to self-govern, 2) Peer-group intensification is moderated by exposure to adults and multiple age groups, and 3) Adults disregard youth less as a result of mutually positive interactions. 96 Recommendations to decrease adolescent alienation have centered on these ideas of integration, both within peer groups and across age groups, and increased decision-making (e.g., Calabrese, 1987a; Rafky, 1979; Newmann, 1981). Rafky (1979) and Zimmerman (1990) have laid a foundation suggesting that increased political efficacy and community involvement are effective in reduce alienation and promoting empowerment. Such involvement may hold the key to improving adolescent alienation across many settings. Calabrese (1987a) has also suggested that adolescent alienation can be reduced by integrating adolescents with multiple age groups while also alleviating the segregation between peers. Additional recommendations offered by Calabrese include efforts to assimilate adolescents into society to provide a sense of meaning and contribution to society, and promote enfranchisement by giving opportunities for adolescents to actively participate in the decision- making processes that affect them. Similar, but more extensive recommendations were offered by Newmann (1981). He has suggested that the main goals in reducing adolescent alienation at school should be to promote individuality, communality, and integration. To accomplish these goals he has suggested that 1) students should have increased voluntary choices so that "students and their parents voluntarily develop and attend schools whose education purposes they share” (p.551), 2) schools develop greater clarity and consistency in their goals, 3) school size be maximized to provide the resources of larger schools but the sustained individual contact of smaller schools, 4) students be provided opportunities to participate in school policy development and management, 5) teachers and students have more cooperative and extensive roles to increase trust and mutual, rather than top-down, accountability, and that 6) school work should be more meaningful and integrated with the unique skills and interests of individual students. The recommendations of these authors, in combination with the present research, suggest that families, educators, community leaders, youth workers, and others interested in the welfare of youth take an 97 active role in promoting environments that are conducive to participatory decision-making, recognition of individual contributions, and meaningful relationships across multiple age groups and roles. Appendices Appendix A 98 Appendix.A Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Classroom Environment Scales19 Factor 1 - Classroom order items (correlation matrix) 11 19 27 5 21 9 7 15 23 30 9 25 31 F1 F2 F3 F4 11 41 49 44 32 14 21 11 11 26 18 13 6 4 64 33 31 12 19 49 59 53 31 7 18 18 15 25 21 10 16 8 77 28 38 18 27 44 53 48 27 7 22 10 7 17 21 5 18 ~1 69 28 26 12 Factor 2 ~ Classroom rule clarity items (correlation matrix) 11 19 27 5 21 9 7 15 23 30 9 25 31 F1 F2 F3 F4 5 32 31 27 31 32 48 28 13 36 28 18 18 8 43 55 50 24 21 14 7 7 32 35 51 12 -5 21 14 29 24 15 13 59 20 37 9 21 18 22 48 51 74 15 -7 23 28 30 30 22 29 87 28 44 Factor 3 - Classroom control items (correlation matrix) 11 19 27 5 21 9 7 15 23 30 9 25 31 F1 F2 F3 F4 7 11 18 10 28 12 15 25 29 29 22 7 12 5 19 27 50 13 15 11 15 7 13 -5 -7 29 20 26 19 ~14 ~11 ~11 16 0 45 ~19 23 26 25 17 36 21 23 29 26 42 39 11 17 18 32 40 65 25 30 18 21 21 28 14 28 22 19 39 25 6 10 13 29 35 50 16 Factor 4 - Classroom innovation items (correlation matrix) 11 19 27 5 21 9 7 15 23 30 9 25 31 F1 F2 F3 F4 9 13 10 5 18 29 30 7 ~14 11 6 56 42 41 13 38 25 6 16 18 18 24 30 12 ~11 17 10 42 31 30 19 36 13 56 31 4 8 ~1 8 15 22 5 -11 18 13 41 30 30 5 22 12 54 U" s] U" Factor Correlations 11 19 27 5 21 9 7 15 23 30 9 25 31 F1 F2 F3 F4 F1 64 77 69 43 13 29 19 16 32 29 13 19 5 100 43 45 20 F2 33 28 28 55 59 87 27 0 40 35 38 36 22 43 100 49 52 F3 31 38 26 50 20 28 50 45 65 50 5 13 12 45 49 100 16 F4 12 18 12 24 37 44 13 ~19 25 16 75 56 54 20 52 16 100 Average correlation within clusters: F1 .4866667 F2 .4366666 F3 .2733333 F4 .3766667 Standard score coefficient alphas: F1 .740 F2 .699 F3 .601 F4 .644 19Itemnumbers correspond to questionnaire items and bold items denote item loadings on the appropriate factor. Appendix B 99 Appendix B Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Family Environment Scales2° Factor 1 ~ Family organization (correlation matrix) 32 38 42 47 35 40 45 50 F1 F2 32 14 24 15 25 -6 17 -6 17 38 13 38 24 39 38 28 -14 0 -2 13 62 -2 42 15 38 29 29 -13 6 -8 13 53 -1 47 25 28 29 29 -2 16 8 26 53 28 Factor 2 - Family control (correlation matrix) 32 38 42 47 35 40 45 50 F1 F2 35 -6 ~14 ~13 -2 14 20 11 19 ~17 38 40 17 0 6 16 20 26 12 29 19 51 45 ~6 ~2 -8 8 11 12 7 15 -4 26 50 17 13 13 26 19 29 15 30 33 54 Factor Correlations 32 38 42 47 35 40 45 50 F1 F2 F1 38 62 53 53 ~17 19 ~4 33 100 18 F2 13 ~2 -1 28 38 51 26 54 18 100 Average correlation within clusters: F1 .265 F1 .1766667 Standard score coefficient alphas: F1 .591 F2 .462 2°Itemnumbers correspond to questionnaire items and bold items denote item loadings on the appropriate factor. Appendix C 100 Appendix C Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Dean Alienation Scale21 Factor 1 — Dean alienation (correlation matrix) 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 59 60 F1 51 20 59 5 12 15 11 25 21 19 45 52 59 15 15 6 8 8 23 14 15 39 57 25 23 8 15 28 29 22 20 25 47 59 21 14 16 27 25 20 20 28 50 53 60 19 15 16 23 27 38 25 50 36 60 Factor Correlations F1 45 39 32 40 47 51 47 53 60 100 Average correlation within cluster: F1 .2122222 Standard score coefficient alpha: F2 .708 1“Item numbers correspond to questionnaire items and bold items denote item. loadings on the appropriate factor. Appendix D 101 Appendix D Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Alienation & Empowerment Scale22 School School Family Family Alienation Empowerment Alienation Empowerment (SA) (SE) (EA) (FE) School Alienation Items 6 .63 —.47 .26 -.27 12 .60 ~.65 .39 -.42 16 .66 -.53 .27 -.30 20 .71 -.68 .38 -.35 26 .76 ~.61 .34 ~.29 School Empowerment Items 10 ~.47 .65 ~.24 .34 14 ~.36 .54 ~.28 .42 18 ~.49 .55 ~.21 .29 22 -.46 .61 -.34 .48 28 ~.74 .55 ~.25 .32 Family Alienation Items 34 .37 ~.35 .77 ~.69 37 .25 ~.28 .54 ~.52 43 .31 ~.21 .55 ~.50 48 .40 ~.43 .83 ~.74 49 .26 ~.21 .57 ~.39 Family Empowerment Items 33 ~.29 .45 —.57 .59 36 ~.38 .45 -.58 .74 44 -.34 .42 -.64 .74 Correlations among scales (SA) .100 ~.87 .49 ~.49 (SE) ~.87 .100 ~.45 .64 (FA) .49 ~.45 .100 ~.87 (FE) -.49 .64 ~.87 .100 Average correlation within clusters: F1 .451 F2 .337 F3 .424 F4 .4766667 Standard score coefficient alphas: F1 .804 F2 .718 F3 .786 F4 .732 ”Item.numbers correspond with number on questionnaire and bold items denote item loadings on the appropriate factor. Appendix E 102 nN.o 00.0: 0N.o o~.o n~.0 00.0I 00.0 aua>an<.Eah 000.H n~.0 no.0: «v.0 va.0 0H.o 0H.0| 00.0 awn>auut com ooo.a >0.0I na.o mu.0| 0H.on nH.0| v0.0 00.0 spasm Eon «no.0 no.0 00.0 ma.o hH.0 ma.o v0.0 00.0 codd< Sam Noe.o 0v.o: vN.o v~.o| NH.on «H.01 no.0 00.0 IOQEm com oar.0 vn.o 0H.oi 0N.o 0a.o mH.o no.0: 00.0 coda< com hoo.o en.o «~.oi mN.o oa.o HH.0 «0.0 00.0 cond< coco noh.o nH.o 0H.0i v0.0a 00.0: NH.0| No.0 00.0 >occH condo vvo.o nN.o no.0: v0.0 no.0 no.0 no.0: 00.0 Honucou woman Hoo.0 00.0 HH.0| HH.0| No.0 v0.0: no.0 00.0 Odom onoao mmm.0 NH.0 no.0: 00.0 v~.0 vN.o no.0 00.0 novno nnoao ove.o hH.0 00.0 Na.0l No.0 no.0 00.0 00.0 Honucoo Emu Nov.o mm.H wo.o No.0 0.10 0H.o mo.OI 00.0 HOUnO Emu Hmm.o oo.o oo.H nv.o| no.0 no.0 00.0 NH.0 noocoun< ooo.H No.0 nv.0I oo.a nN.0I no.0! ETC! hN.OI (m0 ooo.a wH.o 5.0.0 MN.OI oo.H no.0 no.0! 00.0 02 000.." ma.o no.0 no.0: no.0 00.H no.0: No.0 ocnno 000.H wo.on no.0 va.0l no.0! mo.on oo.H no.0 nova—00 000.." 00.0 NH.0 pN.0i 00.0 «0.0 no.0 oo.a vcoauom 000.H noUnO Eflw moocomn< (mo oo< oomnu noncow UCOduom noaoom 6 conuoscouufi nou vouuonnoo uoaoom no coauodonnoonoucH m 53:09? 103 -.o no.0 oo.o 00.0 no.0: 00.0.. 5325004 Bah ooo.H 0H.o no.0! No.0I v0.0! OH.OI 00.0: %u«>«UQfl 80m ooo.H 3.0: .30.. n~.0| nn.0i 3.0: end 3095 Bob «no.0 no.0 3.0 8.0 ~10 00.0 pad: 00.32 Sum «no.0 hv.0l Hm.0I Nn.0I no.0: wm.on nH.o SOQEM com mHh.o 00.0 mm.o bd.o mn.o Hm.o $0.0: cmad< com hom.o 3.4 3.0 00.0 mad and mn.ou can: coco 02.0 HN.0 mm.H ma.o no.0 NN.o no.0 >occH nmaao vvo.o no.0 mad not" 0v.0 210 :0 anacoo 23.8 30.0 mH.o mm.o 0v.o Mv.H «v.0 no.0 masm nmmao mmm.o an.o NN.0 mv.o Nv.o mm.d wo.ou nuvno QQMHU Ov0.o mn.0n no.0 3.0 00.0 00.0.. wan Honucoo Emu vao on.0 nH.o MN.o no.0 Nd.o PH.o novno Emu Hmm.o HN.0I 0H.0I Ho.OI HH.0| no.0: oo.o m00cmufl< ooo.a 0N.o v0.0: 00.0 HH.0I 00.0 NH.0I (m0 ooo.H OH.0 00.0I no.0 No.0 vN.o No.0 0nd ooo.H HH.0 NH.0I no.0 v0.0l vN.o Ho.o ovmnw ooo.H No.0 No.0 H0.0I no.0 «0.0 00.0 novcoo ooo.H 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 Ucoaumm ooo.H coda cmoo >occH unmao Honucoo nnmao oasm unsao noUnO cacao Honucou Emu noaoom U “Divan—“003‘ HON UOHOOHHOU uflddom HO GOHUEHOHHOOHOUQH :0 . uaou. a mason: 104 00.H v0.0 on.ol m~.o n~.o| 0N.0 >ua>auu<.5um 000.H v0.0 00.H m~.0n nn.0 nn.ou v~.o Mun>nuu< com ooo.H on.oi m~.o| en.a h0.oi v0.0 no.0: 3005a Emu an.0 n~.0 nn.o >0.on 0~.H on.0| no.0 co«n<.8om Nah.o H~.0i nH.on «0.0 on.0n mn.n no.0: spasm com 0Hh.0 0~.o vm.o no.0: nv.o no.0: v~.H cond< com h00.o NN.0 0H.0 hm.oi no.0 rv.0n 00.0 cona< coco moh.o no.0 no.0: n~.on Ha.o «0.0- mn.o >ocCH ouoao vvo.o 00.0 «0.0: nN.0n ho.0 ~n.0n hH.0 Honucoo unwao H00.0 no.0 v0.0: nn.o| NH.0 no.0: mn.o onsm muono mmo.o no.0: 0H.0i ha.ou >0.o on.0i an.0 noono woman ov>.0 00.0: 00.0: 0N.o 0H.ou nH.0 00.0: Honucoo Emu N0v.0 n~.0 n~.0 bo.on hw.o mv.0i vn.o noono sum amm.o no.0: no.0: nH.o 00.0 vN.o mn.o| ooocomnd 000.H 0N0 «v.0 mN.0I 0.70 vm.0| 0N0 <00 000.4" 0N.0 vH.0 0H.0n en.o «n.0n on.o 00¢ ooo.n nm.o ma.o nH.o| ma.0 «n.0u mH.o ooono 000.H 00.0: ma.0u v0.0 v0.0 no.0 no.0: noncoo 000.a 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 Uconwnom 000...” >un>nuo< Emu >un>nuo<.com zodem Emu conn<.Emm zodEm com conH¢.com noamom o scandacouufl now vouoonnoo noesum Ho coauodonnounoucH «6.96000 u Xavcognfl Appendix F 105 Appendix F Measures of.Alienation Dean.Alienation 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. The world in which we live is basically a friendly place.* People are just naturally friendly and helpful.* I don't get to visit (meet with people) as often as I like. There are so many decision that have to be made today that sometimes I could just "blow up." There is little chance for getting ahead in life unless you get a break. We're so regimented (structured into a routine) that there's not much room for choice even in personal matters. The future looks very dismal. Everything is relative, and there just aren't any definite rules to live by. I often wonder what the meaning of life really is. The only thing one can be sure of today is that we can be sure of nothing. Family Alienation My family doesn't care what I think. Nothing is important to me at home. I can't be myself at home. My parents do not let me make my own decisions. I don't really make a difference in my family. I feel like a fake when I'm around my family. School Alienation 6. 12. 16. 20. 24. 26. At school other people are running my life. I don't really make a difference at school. I can't be myself at school. The people at my school don't care about what I think. While I'm at school life seems to have no purpose. I have to be somebody I'm not while I'm at school. *Items have been reflected Appendix G 106 Appendix G Measures of Empowerment Family Empowerment 33. 36. 41. 44. 46. I fit in with my family. I take an active role in making decisions in my family. I don't play games with my family in order to fit in. My opinions are important to my family. I get excited thinking about the future while I'm talking with my family. School Empowerment 8. 10. 14. 18. 22. 28. I have important things to do at school. My opinions are important to the people around me in school. When I am at school it seems that life is full of meaning and purpose. I fit in with the people around me at school. I take an active role in making decisions at school. .At school I can be who I want to be. Appendix H 107 Appendix H Measures of Classroom Environment Order 4. This is a well organized class.* 11. Students are almost always quiet in this class.* 19. Students fool around a lot in this class. 27. This class is often in an uproar. Rule Clarity 5. There is a clear set of rules for students to follow in this class.* 13. Rules in this class seem to change a lot. 21. The teacher explains what will happen if a student breaks a rule.* 29. The teacher explains what the rules are.* Teacher Control 7. There are very few rules to follow in this class 15. If a student breaks a rule in this class, she or he's sure to get in trouble.* 23. The teacher is not very strict. 30. Students can get into trouble with the teacher for talking when they're not supposed to.* Innovation 9 New ideas are always being tried out here in class.* 17. What students do in class is very different on different days.* 25. New and different ways of teaching are not tried very often in this class. 31. The teacher likes students to try unusual projects.* *Items have been reflected Appendix I 108 Appendix I Measures of Family Environment Order & Organization 32. Activities in our family are pretty carefully planned.* 38. We are generally very neat and orderly at home.* 42. It's often hard to find things when you need them in our household. 47. Being on time is very important in our family.* Control 35. Family members are rarely ordered around. 40. There are very few rules to follow in our family. 45. There is one family member at home who makes most of the decisions.* 50. There are set ways of doing things at home.* *Items have been reflected Appendix J 109 Appendix J Measures of Involvement School Activities 61. a. b. C. 9' P'D'O ram Which of the following activities do you enjoy at school? Academic activities (such as debate, math, science club). Volunteer helpers (such as school monitor, patrol, tutoring, per counselors, office aid). Helping with a fund—raising event (such as selling candy, magazines, and other things to help raise money for the school). .Athletic teams (such as volleyball, track, football, basketball, cheerleading, etc.). Musical activities (like band or choir). Debate team, drama, or theater. School clubs (like chess, computer, vocational, mathematics). School newspaper or yearbook. Student government. Family Activities 62. a. b. c. d. e. f. 9. Which of the following activities do you enjoy with your family? Taking vacations. Playing sports or games. Going to athletic events. Watching T.V. or videos. Going to cultural events (such as museums, plays, etc.). Being at family get-togethers (such as holiday celebrations, family reunions, parties, etc.). Going to social events (such as a picnic, party, club, festival, etc.) List of References List of References Albas, D., Albas, C., & McCluskey, K. (1978). Anomie, social class and drinking behavior of high school students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 39(5), 910-913. Alessandri, S. M., & Wozniak, R. H. (1989). Perception of the family environment and intrafamily agreement in belief concerning the adolescent. Journal of Early Adolescence, 9(1-2), 67-81. Avi-Itzhak, T. E. (1987). Organizational characteristics of Israeli Arab secondary schools: Factors affecting student alienation from school. The High School Journal, 70, 141-144. Baumrind, D. (1987). A.developmental perspective on adolescent risk taking in contemporary America. New Directions for Child Development, 37, 93—125. Boggiano, A. K., & Katz, P. (1991). Maladaptive achievement patterns in students: The role of teachers' controlling strategies. Journal of Social Issues, 47(4), 35-51. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). The origins of alienation. Scientific American, 231 (2), 53-61. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513-531. Burbach, H. J. (1972, April). An empirical study of powerlessness among high school students. The High School Journal, 343-354. Calabrese, R. L. (1987a). Adolescence: A.growth period conducive to alienation. Adolescence, 22, 929-938. Calabrese, R. L. (1987b). A Comparative analysis of the effects of alienation on public and private school females. The Journal of Psychology, 121(3), 237-242. Calabrese, R. L. (1988). The effects of family factors on levels of adolescent alienation. The High School Journal, 71(4), 187-191. Calabrese, R. L., & Schumer, H. (1986). The effects of service activities on adolescent alienation. Adolescence, 21(83), 675-687. Calabrese, R. L., & Seldin, C. (1986). Adolescent alienation: An analysis of the female response to the secondary school environment. The High School Journal, 69, 120-125. Caplan, N., & Nelson, S. D. (1973). On being useful: The nature and consequences of psychological research on social problems. American Psychologist, 28(3), 199-211. Coleman, J.S. (1961). The Adolescent Society. New York: Free Press. Dean, D. G. (1961). Alienation: Its meaning and measurement. American Sociological Review, 26, 753- 758. 110 111 DeMan, A. F. (1982). Autonomy-control variation in child rearing and level of alienation in young adults. The Journal of Psychology, 112, 71-78. Dinning, W. D., & Berk, L. A. (1989). The children of alcoholics screening test: Relationship to sex, family environments, and social adjustment in adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45(2), 335— 339. Downs, A. C. (1990). Perceptions of physical appearance and adolescents' social alienation. Psychological Reports, 67(3 Part 2), 1305-1306. Eccles, J. S., Miller Buchanan, C., Flanagan, C., Fuligni, C. M., & Yee, D. (1991). Control versus autonomy during early adolescence. Journal of Social Issues, 47(4), 53-68. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York: Norton. Farber, S. 5., Felner, R. D., & Primavera, J. (1985). Parental separation/divorce and adolescents: An examination of factors mediating adaptation. American Journal of Community Psycholggy, 13(2), 171-185. Felner, R. D., Aber, M. S., Primavera, J., a Cauce, A. M. (1985). Adaptation and vulnerability in high risk adolescents: An examination of environmental mediators. American Journal of Community Psychology, 55(4), 365-379. Fox, R., Rotatori, A. F., Macklin, F., Green, H., & Fox, T. (1983). Socially maladjusted adolescents perceptions of their families. Psychological Reports, 52, 831-834. Friedrich, W. N., Reams, R., & Jacobs, J. H. (1988). Sex differences in depression in early adolescents. Psychological Reports, 62, 475-481. Fromm, E. (1964). Sane Society. In Sykes (Ed.). Alienation. (Vol. 1). New York: Braziller. Galbo, J. J. (1983). Adolescents' perceptions of significant adults. Adolescence, 18(70), 417-427. Gladding, S. T. (1977). Psychological anomie and religious identity in two adolescent populations. Psychological Reports, 41(2), 419-424. Gleser, G., Winget, C., & Seligman, R. (1979). Content scaling of affect in adolescent speech samples. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 2(3), 283-297. Hartley, M. C., & Hoy, W. K. (1972). ”Openness" of school climate and alienation of high school students. California Journal of Educational Research, 23(1), 17-24. Hendrix, V. L., Sederberg, C. H., Miller, V. L. (1990). Correlates of commitment/alienation among high school seniors. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 23(3), 129-135. Holmgren, C., Fitzgerald, B. J., & Carman, R. s. (1983). .Alienation and alcohol use by American Indian and Caucasian high school students. The Journal of Social Psychology, 120(1), 139-140. 112 Hoy, W. K. (1972). Dimensions of alienation and characteristics of public high schools. Interchange, 3, 38-50. Hunter, J. E., & Cohen, S. H. (1969). PACKAGE: A.system of computer routines for the analysis of correlational data. Educational and Psycholggical Measurement, 29, 697-700. Jackson, D. W. (1973). Alienation and identity-role diffusion in late adolescence. The Journal of Psychology, 83, 251-255. Jackson, D. W. (1974). Stage factors relating alienation and self- role diffusion. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 5(2), 269-274. Jones, W. Jr., (1977-78). Interaction effects of students, drugs and alienation. Journal of Drug Education,,7(3), 271-278. Kids count data book: Stategprofiles of child well-being. (1993). Washington, D. C.: The Annie E. Casey Foundation a Center for the Study of Social Policy. Kids count in michigan 1993 data book: County profiles of child and family well-being. (1993). Lansing, Michigan: Michigan League for Human Services. Kieffer, C.H. (1984). Citizen empowerment: A.developmental perspective. Prevention in Human Services, 3, 9-36. Kinkel R. J., Bailey, C. W., & Josef, N. C. (1989). Correlates of adolescent suicide attempts: Alienation, drugs and social background. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 34(3), 85-96. Kleinman, S. L., Handal, P. J., Enos, D., Searight, H. R., 5 Ross, M. J. (1989). Relationship between perceived family climate and adolescent adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18(4), 351- 359. Kulka, R. A., Kahle, L. R., & Klingel, D. M. (1982). Aggression, deviance, and personality adaptation as antecedents and consequences of alienation and involvement in high school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 11(3), 261-279. LaTorre, R. A., Yu, L., Fortin, L. & Marrache, M. (1983). Gender- role adoption and sex as academic psychological risk factors. Sex Roles, 2(11), 1127-1136. Lewin, K. (1935). A Dynamic Theory of Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48(12), 1181-1209. Mackey, J., & Ahlgren, A. (1977). Dimensions of adolescent alienation. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(2), 219-232. Maddi, S. R. (1972). Personality Theories. rev. ed. Homewood, Ill: Dorsey Press. Madonna, 8., Bailey, G. K., & Wesley, A. L. (1990). Classroom environment and locus of control in identifying high and low self- concept in fourth- and fifth-graders. Psychological Reports, 66, 1152- 1154. 113 Margalit, M., & Eysenck, S. (1990). Prediction of coherence in adolescence: Gender differences in social skills, personality, and family climate. Journal of Research in Personality, 24, 510-521. Miller, P. Y., & Simon, W. (1974). Adolescent sexual behavior: Context and change. Social Problems, 22(1), 58-76. Merton, R. (1968). Social structure and anomie. In M. Lefton (Ed.), Approaches to deviance. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts. Mohanty, N. (1984). Social and psychological correlates of alienation among adolescents. Psychological Studies, 29(2), 147-151. Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1981). Family Environment Scale Manual. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press. Nelson, G. (1984). The relationship between dnmensions of classroom and family environments and the self-concept, satisfaction, and achievement of grade 7 and 8 students. Journal of Community Psychology, 12, 276-287. Newmann, F. M. (1981). Reducing student alienation in high schools: Implications of theory. Harvard Educational Review, 51(4), 546- 564. Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory. (2nd Ed.). New York: McGraw—Hill. Owie, I. (1988). Alienation and the use of psychogenic drugs among adolescents in Nigeria: A report. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 55(2), 37-40. Rafalides, M., & Hoy, W. (1971). Student Sense of alienation and pupil control orientation of high schools. The High School Journal, 55, 101-111. Rafky, D. (1979). School rebellion: A research note. Adolescence, 55(55), 451-464. Rappaport, J. (1981). In praise of paradox: A.social policy of empowerment over prevention. American Journal of Community Psycholggy, 5, 1-25. Reimanis, G. (1974). Anomie, crime, childhood memories, and development of social interest. Journal of Individual Psychology, 55(1), 53-58. Rode, A. (1971). Perceptions of parental behavior among alienated adolescents. Adolescence, 6(21), 19-38. Rogers, A. & Gilligan, C. (1988) Translating girls voices': Two lan uages of development. Harvard University Graduate School of Education, Harvard Project on the psychology of women and the development of girls. 42-43 Schimek, J. G., & Milstein Meyer, R. (1975). Dimensions of alienation and pathology. Psychological Reports, 37, 727-732. Seeman, M. (1959). On the meaning of alienation. American Sociological Review, 24, 783-791. Shearin, W. (1982). The relationship between student alienation and extent of faculty agreement on pupil control ideology. The High School Journal, 66, 32-35. 114 Sinha, A. K., Singh, A. P., & Kumari, A. (1988). Parent evaluation as a function of sex and alienation. Indian Journal of Current Psychological Research, 3(1), 29-33. Stanley, J. C. (1975). Intellectual precocity. The Journal of Special Education, 9(1), 29-44. Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conflict, and harmony in the family relationship. In S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.). At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent. (pp. 255-276). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Trickett, E. J., & Moos, R. H. (1974). Classroom Environment Scale. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press. U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (1992). Eggcher survey on safe, disciplined, and drgg-free schools. In Digest of Education Statistics. In L. Schmittroth, Ed. (1994). Statistical record of children. Detroit: Gale Research, p. 197. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1993). Use of selected substances in the past month by youths 12-17 years of age and young adults 18-25 years of age according to age, sex, race, and Hispanic origin: United States selected years 1974-1991. Healthy United States 1992 and Healthy People 2000 Review. In L. Schmittroth, Ed. (1994). Statistical record of children. Detroit: Gale Research, p. 310. U.S. Department of Justice, Federal Bureau of Investigation. (1992, August 30). Percent changes in juvenile arrest rates for crimes related to violence, United States, 1990 over 1980. Crime in the United §tates 1991: Uniform Crime Reports, Table 5.1,,p.289. In L. Schmittroth, Ed. (1994). Statistical record of children. Detroit: Gale Research, p. 793. Verma, B. P. (1990). Alienation in relation to academic achievement of adolescents. Indiana Journal of Psychometry and Education, 21(1), 29-35. Warner, R., & Hansen, J. (1970). The relationship between alienation and other demographic variables among high school students. The High School Journal, 54, 202-210. Wilkerson, J., Protinsky, H. D. Jr., Maxwell, J. W., & Lentner, M. (1982). Alienation and ego identity in adolescents. Adolescence, 17(65), 133-139. Wright, S., & Cowen, E. L. (1982). Student perception of school environment and its relationship to mood, achievement, popularity, and adjustment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 10(6), 1982. Wynne, E. (1978). Behind the discipline problem: Youth suicide as a measure of alienation. Phi Delta Kappan, 59, 307-315. Young, T. J. (1985). Adolescent suicide: The clinical manifestation of alienation. The High School Journal, 69, 55-60. Zimmerman, M. A. (1990). Toward a theory of learned hopefulness: A structural model analysis of participation and empowerment. Journal of Research in Personality, 24, 71-86. Zimmerman, M..A., & Rappaport, J. (1988). Citizen participation, perceived control, and psychological empowerment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 16(5), 725-750. 115 Zimmerman, M. A., Israel, B. A. Schulz, A., & Checkoway, B. (1992). Further explorations in empowerment theory: An empirical analysis of psychological empowerment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 20(6), 707-728. "Iiitttlttitttttilttt“