Aw. .u 5&5. 3.. mafia... :1, ., H .353; . $.25 % h 1: .4: wtfinx \\4» 1! . 1.. .313... . .v:-0.u: I. ‘ «P‘Ffln‘ I. ll.|V tfljfililnv '|;u: ... Phi-‘5 rug '3‘ IUIJHHIIHIII!UIHWIHIHIUHIHUIHHHIHHIJHlUll 01410 3166 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Family Factors in the Psychosocial Adaptation of Children and Adolescents with Short Stature presented by Carol Cracchiolo Laub has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. mgmem Psychology Major professor/ fw/zc %/// 7/r Date /7/75/‘// MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE ll RETURN BOX to remove thle checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before dete due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU le An Affirmative Maud Opportunity lnetltuion WINS-a1 FAMILY FACTORS IN THE PSYCHOSOCIAL ADAPTATION OF CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS WITH SHORT STATURE By Carol Cracchiolo Laub A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1995 ABSTRACT FAMILY FACTORS IN THE PSYCHOSOCIAL ADAPTATION OF CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS WITH SHORT STATURE By Carol Cracchiolo Laub Children significantly below average in height are at risk for poorer psychosocial outcomes than children of normal height. However, recent investigations have yielded inconsistent results regarding the functioning of children with short stature (SS). The present study utilized both questionnaire and interview data to assess the psychosocial adjustment of children with SS (at or below the 5th percentile of height for age). Relationships between the family environment, children’s and parents’ self-worth, experiences related to stature, coping strategies, and child outcomes were also examined. Subjects were 33 children with SS and their parent(s), and a matched comparison group of 33 healthy children of normal stature (NS) and their parent(s). Compared to children with NS, children with SS were rated by their parents as having more behavior problems and lower social competence. Children with SS rated themselves as having lower global self-worth and athletic competence, and as less satisfied with their physical appearance than children with NS. Girls and boys with SS had similar levels of behavior and social problems. However, girls with SS participated in fewer extracurricular activities, and rated themselves as less athletically competent and less satisfied with their physical appearance than boys with SS and children with NS. Younger children with SS reported higher levels of self- competence and global self-worth than older children with SS, but there were no age differences in parent-reported behavior problems or competence. Devaluing physical appearance was related to greater self-competence and higher global self-worth in children with SS. The family environments of children with SS did not differ significantly from those of children with NS, although families of children with SS described themselves as somewhat less supportive. Family support was associated with better child adjustment, whereas family control and conflict were associated with poorer child outcomes. Fathers’ coping, global self-worth, and the impact of their own stature were more strongly related to child outcomes than mothers’. A model of stress and coping with SS predicted 46% of the variance in child behavior problems. Methodological considerations in the assessment of coping and implications for clinical interventions are discussed. To Rick and Emily, my greatest inspirations. iv ACKNOWLEDGNIENTS I would like to extend my deepest gratitude to the Chairpersons of my dissertation committee, John McKinney and Elizabeth Seagull, for their enduring support, guidance, and confidence in me. I would also like to thank the members of my dissertation committee, Hiram Fitzgerald, Robert Caldwell, and Bruce Wilson, for their willingness to contribute to the development and completion of this work. Bruce Wilson and Kathleen Seitz deserve special recognition for conceptualizing the original seeds for this research and allowing me to cultivate them into my own. I would also like to thank my family and friends, who supported me throughout this project with their love, encouragement, confidence, and assistance. Most of all, I want to express my appreciation to my husband, Rick, for all he has done in order to make this possible. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ...................................... viii LIST OF FIGURES ....................................... x CHAPTER 1 - Review of the Literature .......................... 1 Introduction ...................................... 1 Social Stigma and Stature Stereotypes ...................... 6 Self-Image, Self-Esteem, and Self-Competence ................ 10 Juvenilization: Living up to Expectations? ............... 12 Psychosocial Adjustment to SS ........................... 16 Psychosocial Outcomes in Childhood and Adolescence ........ 16 Psychosocial Outcomes in Adulthood .................. 26 Family Factors and Child Adjustment to SS .............. 28 Transactional Stress and Coping Model ..................... 36 Limitations of Previous Research on SS ..................... 40 Overview and Research Question ......................... 42 CHAPTER 2 - Research Hypotheses ........................... 44 CHAPTER 3 - Method .................................... 46 Subjects ......................................... 46 Procedures ....................................... 49 Measures ........................................ 52 Demographic Parameters/Instrumental Family Resources ...... 52 Family Mediational Processes ....................... 53 Child Mediational Processes ........................ 56 Child Adjustment .............................. 58 Semi-Structured Interview ......................... 60 CHAPTER 4 - Results .................................... 64 Hypotheses 1 and 2: Group Differences ..................... 65 Hypothesis 3: Gender Differences ......................... 68 Hypothesis 4: Age Differences .......................... 73 vi Page Hypothesis 5: Family History of SS ....................... 74 Hypothesis 6: Devaluing Physical Appearance ................. 75 Hypotheses 7, 8, 9, and 10 : Family Factors in Child Outcomes ..... 77 Hypothesis 11: The Stress and Coping Model ................ 82 CHAPTER 5 - Discussion .................................. 97 Group Differences .................................. 97 Gender Differences .................................. 100 Age Differences .................................... 101 The Impact of a Family History of SS ...................... 103 The Importance of Physical Appearance ..................... 105 Family Factors in Child Outcomes ........................ 106 Coping ......................................... 110 The Stress and Coping Model ........................... 113 Study Limitations and Future Directions ..................... 117 APPENDICES A. Frequently Used Acronyms ......................... 122 B. Recruitment Letter to Parents ........................ 123 C. UCRIHS Letters of Approval ........................ 124 D. Informed Consent Form ........................... 126 E. Questionnaire Packet Letter ......................... 127 F. Child and Family Information Questionnaire ............... 128 G. Semi-Structured Interview for Parents ................... 131 H. Semi-Structured Interview for Children and Adolescents ....... 134 I. Coping Coding Manual ............................ 137 J. Sample Coding Sheet ............................. 140 LIST OF REFERENCES .................................. 141 vii Table 10 11 LIST OF TABLES Page Group Comparisons for the Child Behavior Checklist .............. 66 Group Comparisons for the Self-Perception Profile ............... 67 One-Way Analysis of Variance (AN OVA) for Stature X Gender Groups and CBCL Behavior Problem Scales .............. 70 One-Way Analysis of Variance (AN OVA) for Stature X Gender Groups and CBCL Competence Scales .................. 71 One-Way Analysis of Variance (AN OVA) for Stature X Gender Groups and Self-Perception Scales .................... 72 Pearson Correlations Between the Family Environment Factors and Child Outcome Measures ....................... 79 Pearson Correlation Coefficients for the Diagnostic/ Demographic and Child Adjustment Variables of the Stress and Coping Model ............................... 83 Pearson Correlation Coefficients for the Family Mediational and Child Adjustment Variables of the Stress and Coping Model ............................. 84 Pearson Correlation Coefficients for the Child Mediational and Child Adjustment Variables of the Stress and Coping Model ............................. 85 Summary of the Multiple Hiearchical Regression Scores for Total Behavior Problems ........................ 89 Summary of the Multiple Hiearchical Regression Scores for Social Competence ............................ 90 viii Table Page 12 Summary of the Multiple Hiearchical Regression Scores for Total Behavior Problems: Coping Removed from the Model ............................... 94 13 Summary of the Multiple Hiearchical Regression Scores for Academic Competence: Coping Removed from the Model ............................... 95 14 Summary of the Multiple Hiearchical Regression Scores for Social Competence: Coping Removed from the Model ............................... 96 ix LIST OF FIGURES Page Stress and Coping Model for Short Stature .............. 39 Variables and Measures .......................... 63 CHAPTER 1 Review of the Literature Introduction The recent increase in the availability of synthetic human growth hormone has led to growing interest in the psychosocial and cognitive functioning of individuals with short stature (SS). Factors related to the expense and possible negative side- effects of treatment with synthetic human growth hormone (GH), make it especially important to clarify the nature and degree of psychosocial benefits of such treatment, particularly for individuals who are not growth hormone deficient, but may respond to such treatment nevertheless (see Appendix A for frequently used acronyms). Short stature is the most frequent reason for referral to pediatric endocrinology clinics and is a "physiological manifestation of growth failure which results from a variety of causes" (Finley, Crouthamel, & Richman, 1982, p. 27). Rieser and Underwood (1990) divide the causes of growth failure and short stature into three broad categories: 1) intrinsic defects of the growing tissues (skeletal dysplasias, autosomal abnormalities, abnormalities of the X chromosome [c.g. , Turner Syndrome], and dysmorphic or primordial dwarfism [c.g. , abnormalities resulting from in utero insults or genetic defects]); 2) abnormalities in the environment of growing tissues (nutritional insufficiency, renal disease, cardiac disease, gastrointestinal disease, poorly controlled diabetes, and vitamin D resistant rickets or other metabolic problems); and 3) endocrine abnormalities (thyroid deficiency, growth hormone deficiency, and glucocorticoid excess [c.g. , adrenal tumors]). Growth 2 hormone deficiency (GHD) is one of the most widely studied growth disorders and is caused by a deficiency of growth hormone secretion secondary to pituitary gland malfunction or hypothalamic dysfunction. When growth hormone is the only hormone not secreted by the pituitary, the disorder is labeled isolated growth hormone deficiency (IGHD). When more than one of the pituitary hormones are deficient, the disorder is labeled multiple hormone deficiency (MHD) (Siegel, 1990). In many cases, the etiology of the growth hormone deficiency is unexplained or idiopathic (Siegel, 1990). Constitutional delay (CD) is a condition in which the cause for growth delay is unknown. Children with CD have normal birth weight, early growth failure, subsequent normal growth velocity, height below the 5th percentile in childhood and delayed pubertal onset, but the potential to attain normal adult height (Siegel, 1990). By contrast, familial short stature (FSS) is a condition in which the rate of growth is normal, the bone age is within 2 SD of that expected for age, and significant short stature is associated with genetic inheritance. Regardless of its etiology, short stature has long been regarded as a social handicap which carries with it a significant degree of stigmatization, especially for very short males (Martel & Biller, 1987). In the context of a "heightist" society, short adults face discrimination in being hired for a job, starting salaries, and salary increases (Gillis, 1982). While the stigmatization and juvenilization of short individuals begins in early childhood, these social processes continue throughout adolescence and adulthood (Clopper, MacGillivray, Mazur, Voorhess, & Mills, 1986; Eisenberg, Roth, Bryniarski, & Murray, 1984; Martel & Biller, 1987). Constant 3 exposure to stigmatization and juvenilization is likely to shape the developing self- concepts, personalities, and motivations of short individuals. Studies of children with short stature (SS) suggest that compared to children of normal height, children with SS are more psychosocially immature and withdrawn (Gordon, Crouthamel, Post, & Richman, 1982), have more somatic complaints and less tolerance for frustration (Drotar, Owens, & Gotthold, 1980; Gordon et al., 1982), poorer academic achievement (Siegel, 1990), and experience greater juvenilization in social interactions and overprotection by their parents (Martel & Biller, 1987; Rotnem, Genel, Hintz, & Cohen, 1977). However, there is significant inconsistency in the literature regarding developmental outcomes, and gender, age, and etiological differences among children with SS. Many of these inconsistencies can be attributed in part to methodological flaws. For example, the majority of studies in this field: 1) examine heterogeneous groups of children with SS; 2) rely solely on questionnaire measures from either the parents or children; 3) fail to account for the effects of socioeconomic status; 4) fail to examine family factors affecting the child’s adaptation to SS; and 5) do not provide a group of carefully matched control subjects for comparison. While earlier investigations of children and adolescents with SS indicate that they have significant psychosocial, behavioral, and cognitive deficits (e.g. , Abbott et al., 1982; Gordon et al., 1982; Holmes et al., 1982; Rotnem et al., 1977), more recent studies demonstrate average or elevated global self-esteem in children with SS (Young-Hyman, 1986; 1990). Despite positive global self-esteem, however, specific 4 deficits in cognitive and social functioning have been identified (Siegel, 1990). In addition, of note is the fact that non-clinic populations of children with SS do not demonstrate significantly more psychosocial or behavioral problems than age-matched control children of normal height (Voss, Bailey, Mulligan, Wilkin, & Betts, 1991). The non-clinic children with SS do, however, demonstrate some tendency toward poor concentration and hyperactivity. Differences between clinic-referred and non-clinic populations of children with SS suggest that there is more significant concern about the psychosocial adjustment of those children referred for treatment for SS in either the referring physician, other significant adults (e. g. , parents, teachers, grandparents), or the child him- or herself. While it is likely that children who are referred for treatment for SS do indeed manifest greater psychosocial and behavioral distress, very little attention is given to the factors contributing to their difficulties, and who is most concerned about their growth failure (e.g. , parents, physicians, children). In contrast to the significant efforts which have been made to treat SS with GH, very little work has focused on psychosocial evaluation and intervention with this population. Although treatment with GH is promising, data on adults who were treated with GH as children or adolescents is somewhat equivocal. Many children and parents have unrealistic expectations of treatment with GH and are unhappy with the results (Clopper, 1992; Grew, Stabler, Williams, & Underwood, 1983; Rotnem et al., 1980). Furthermore, in spite of height augmentation, many GHD adults continue to be socially withdrawn, have lower rates of marriage and employment, and experience psychosocial distress 5 (Clopper et al., 1986; Dean et al., 1986; Mitchell et al., 1986). As advances are made in the medical treatment of SS, there is a clear need to increase our efforts to understand better the psychosocial issues which confront children with SS throughout their development into adulthood. In order to treat the patient with SS with a more biopsychosocial approach, it would be valuable to identify which children are at risk for poor psychosocial adaptation to SS. In addition, it would be useful to clarify the ways in which family factors contribute to the developmental outcomes of children with SS. This study combined questionnaire and interview data to examine relationships between the family environment, children’s and parents’ experiences related to height, coping styles, and child psychosocial outcomes. In addition, the present study tested the application of a stress and coping model which links the child’s diagnosis, family instrumental resources, family emotional resources and mediational processes, and child mediational processes, to child psychosocial outcomes. Social Stigm and Stature Stereotypes In their comprehensive review of social stigma and self-esteem, Crocker and Major (1989) define stigmatized social groups as " social categories about which others hold negative attitudes, stereotypes, and beliefs, or which, on average, receive disproportionately poor interpersonal or economic outcomes relative to members of society at large because of discrimination against members of the social category" (p. 609). Stigma theory holds that social disapproval, the hallmark of stigmatization, leads to an individual’s belief that a certain attribute is undesirable to possess. The degree of stigmatization that is assigned to a characteristic is largely determined by its severity, concealability, and disruptfulness. In an attempt to normalize the stigmatizing condition, affected individuals attempt to conceal it and suffer from diminished self-esteem (Westbrook, Bauman, & Shinnar, 1992), which impacts their overall satisfaction with life (Diener, 1984). The degree to which individuals are negatively impacted by social stigma is largely dependent on their ability to cope effectively with the everyday stressors associated with feeling different from what is considered to be "average" or desirable. Ainlay, Coleman, and Becker (1986) propose that stigma must be viewed within a developmental perspective, which takes into account continuities and discontinuities throughout the lifespan as the impact of stigma on personal development waxes and wanes. They state that " even when stigmas no longer continually tug at the individual in daily routines, they linger as memories, reflections of culture that alter people’s behavior and their lives" (p. 7). These writers acknowledge the paradoxical nature 7 of stigma, in that it can be both "dehumanizing and inspiring." The degree to which one overcomes the negative impact of stigma is dependent on numerous factors including: individual personality, physical environment, emotional and material resources, cultural beliefs, education, and social status. Through the process of " normalization" the stigmatized individual adapts to society by reducing the amount of deviation from cultural norms, and by decreasing the emphasis on the stigmatizing condition (Becker & Arnold, 1986). Given that stature is one of the most readily apparent characteristics of an individual, extreme short stature is a stigmatizing feature which is not easily concealed from social interactions and reactions. Within the context of a "heightist" culture, short stature can be considered a social handicap, which shapes interpersonal relationships, the development of self-image and self-competence, coping styles, personality, and motivations. In one of the earlier works concerning stature, Gerling (1939) recognized that society’s view of height is "often out of proportion to its real value" (p.15) . However, he dedicated the majority of his text to techniques of increasing one’s apparent stature through posture, physical exercise, dress, and "stature aids, " thus promoting rather than combating the root of the problem-- "heightism. " Society’s value of height is clearly demonstrated in the stereotypes attributed to persons of varying stature. Adults, adolescents, and children all assign more positive attributes to tall persons than short persons (Biller, 1968; Gillis, 1982; Martel & Biller, 1987; Morrow, 1984; Stafferi, 1967). For example, children from 5 to 13 8 years-old were asked to allocate an assortment of characteristics to 3 human silhouettes of different sizes. The shortest silhouette was described as "unsuccessful," "weak, " " follower, " and "no friends." In contrast, the tallest silhouette was considered " smart, " strong, " "leader, " and "brave" (Clopper, Mazur, & Ellis, 1990). Among college undergraduates, both males and females associated positive attributes with tall men and negative qualities with short men (Martel & Biller, 1987). Tall men were ascribed adjectives such as dominant, optimistic, confident, capable, and masculine. There also exists a perceptual distortion of height based on ascribed social status. For example, adolescent males assigned occupations of higher prestige to individuals perceived as taller (Morrow, 1984), and adults increase their estimates of an individual’s height as the individual’s reported professional status increases (Wilson, 1968). These perceptual biases appear to be even more pronounced in individuals who view themselves as below average in height. For example, Morrow (1984) found that the correlation between assigned occupational prestige and stature was stronger for males who perceived themselves as short. In addition, although short undergraduate males reported feeling as satisfied with their stature as their taller peers, they desired an ideal height which was more discrepant from their actual height than males of average or tall stature (Martel & Biller, 1987). The shorter group of men also placed greater importance on height in obtaining a dating partner and in professional success than average or tall men. The concerns of short males regarding the impact of stature on professional 9 status are based in the reality that height impacts one’s likelihood of being hired for a job, one’s starting salary, and salary increases (see Gillis, 1982). For example, within the same profession, workers of tall stature hold higher positions than those who are shorter, even after accounting for the effects of education and social status (Schumaker, 1982). One of the most poignant examples of heightism in American culture is reflected in our presidential elections. In 80% of the 20 presidential elections from 1904 to 1980 the taller candidate has won (Gillis, 1982). According to Martel and Biller (1987), "Given such a set of environmental circumstances, stereotyping, and cultural expectations, it is clear that a myriad of difficulties confronts the short male in terms of negotiating and solidifying a positive male identity " (p. 6). From the discussion above, it is evident that the majority of studies on social stigma and stereotyping associated with stature has focused on males. This is due in part to cultural preferences regarding stature. Males make global evaluations about their bodies and value large stature and strength. By contrast, females make more specific evaluations about their various body parts and value smallness, except in the breasts (Fisher, 1986; Martel & Biller, 1987). Masculinity is associated with a tall well-muscled physique, whereas femininity is often associated with petite stature. In addition, feelings of being too thin are associated with lower self-esteem in boys, while feeling too heavy is related to decreased self-esteem in girls (Walsh & Wardle, 1993). Adolescent boys’ ideal is to mature early and have a tall and heavy physique, while girls generally prefer to mature at a rate consistent with their peers and to be of 10 average height and weight (Frazier & Lisonbee, 1960). These differences partially account for the higher referral rates of boys for treatment of SS than girls (Clopper, 1992). Both males and females, however, are likely to experience negative consequences as a result of SS, especially as females increasingly take on more competitive roles academically, athletically, and professionally. Self-Image, Self-Esteem, and Self-Competence Global self-esteem has been described as generalized feelings of acceptance, goodness, self-acceptance, self-respect, and worthiness (Crocker & Major, 1989). Self-image, or one’s "multidimensional representation of oneself" (Clopper, 1992) has been associated with one’s self-esteem. Several researchers of the development of self-image assume that exposure to social stigma in general (Crocker & Major, 1989), and that related to SS in particular (Martel & Biller, 1987), has a deleterious effect on self-esteem. However, there is no clear empirical evidence that members of various stigmatized groups (e.g. , racial minorities, individuals with physical or mental handicaps), including those with SS, necessarily suffer from lowered self-esteem. Clopper (1992) contends that "it appears possible for children to have a poor image of their physique due to SS while maintaining high levels of overall self-esteem and self- worth" (p. 28). Crocker & Major (1989) propose three mechanisms by which the self-esteem of stigmatized individuals is protected: 1) attributing negative feedback to one’ s membership in a stigmatized group, 2) comparing oneself with others in the 11 stigmatized group, and 3) selectively devaluing areas in which their group does poorly and valuing those in which their group excels. The first two strategies might be somewhat more difficult for the child with SS to utilize, as they have limited access to others with SS and hence less identification with a SS group as a whole. However, for children with a family history of SS there is greater exposure to other individuals with a similar condition and its attendant difficulties. Identification with the group and social comparison are thus more likely to occur in children with a family history of SS, as is modeling after adult figures who have successfully (or unsuccessfully) weathered the experience of growing up with a stigmatizing condition. On the other hand, for children with no available role models or peers with SS, selective valuing might be the most effective method in protecting their self-esteem. Harter (1986) found that children who valued most the abilities in which they felt especially competent had high self-esteem. It appears that their ability to devalue areas in which they were less proficient helped to augment their feelings of self- worth. Of particular interest to the present study is Harter’s (1986) finding that all children had some difficulty in devaluing physical appearance (as well as cognitive competence and behavioral conduct). Those children who rated themselves low on these domains also had lower global self-esteem. By contrast, athletic ability and social acceptance were more easily discounted if children felt less competent in these domains. Dissatisfaction with one’s perceived physical attractiveness has also been strongly associated with dysphoria in 9-12 year-olds of normal stature (McCabe & 12 Marwit, 1993). In addition, viewing one’s body as ineffective was moderately linked with dysphoric mood. These results were similar for both males and females. Along similar lines, self-image and depression were highly correlated in a sample of adolescents of normal stature, and self-image was the strongest predictor of recovery from depression as compared to age, education, SES, initial levels of depression, and life stressors (Fine, Haley, Gilbert, Forth, 1993). Furthermore, a sense of mastery was one of the most important components of the relationship between depression and self-image. Thus, it is likely that children with SS who feel positively about their physical appearance would have higher global self-esteem and fewer feelings of depression. Conversely, children with SS who are less satisfied with their bodies, may suffer from lower self-esteem and greater symptoms of depression. Bukowsi and Newcomb’s (1983) investigation of peer experiences and identity formation in early adolescence dovetails with the preceding studies. These researchers found that young adolescents’ feelings of self-worth were related to their perceptions of social, cognitive, and physical competence. Among these variables, the strongest relationship was between social competence and global self-esteem. Social competence was more strongly related to physical competence for boys than girls, and social acceptance was more closely associated with perceived social competence for girls than boys. Juvenilization: Living up to Expectations? The visibility of body size makes it a significant feature of self- 13 presentation and likely to be of special importance in the development of a child’s sense of self. Parental judgement of the child whose shape doesn’t measure up to expectations may disturb children’s natural response to eating and physical activity and foster self-criticism (Walsh & Wardle, 1993, p. 1125). Juvenilization, or being treated in accordance with one’s height age rather than chronological age, is one of the most prevalent and persistent difficulties confronting individuals with short stature and delayed maturation. Because stature is one of the most visible and salient characteristics of an individual, others constantly respond to this external stimulus in ways which are developmentally inappropriate. This dissonance between others’ views and expectations and one’s true abilities begins early in life for individuals with short stature, and may influence the individual’s self— perceptions both directly and indirectly, through comparison with others and through the processing of others’ expectations (Eisenberg, Roth, Bryniarski, & Murray, 1984). Although much of the research in this area focuses on adult populations, a few studies have demonstrated that the social effects of stature begin as early as toddlerhood and continue into the elementary school years and beyond (Brackbill & Nevill, 1981; Eisenberg et al., 1984). Eisenberg and her colleagues (1984) conducted three separate studies investigating the relationship between preschoolers’ height, adults’ attributions about the children, and the cognitive and social competencies of the children. In the first study, mothers of preschoolers viewed an array of two 19 month-old boys which 14 were photographed in such a manner so that the boys appeared to be different heights. In the first array, child I appeared taller; in the second, child 2 appeared taller; and in the third, the boys appeared to be of equal height. The mothers were asked to complete a questionnaire which assessed their perceptions of child competency, assignment of punishment for hypothetical transgressions, and estimates of the children’s ages. Significant differences related to height were found for 10 of the 12 items tested, and taller boys were consistently viewed as being more competent. There were no height effects for the assignment of punishment or estimates of the children’s ages. In a second parallel study, mothers of preschoolers were shown stimulus photographs of two girls aged 19 and 20 months (Eisenberg et al., 1984). On 5 of the 12 test items there were significant differences related to height. In each case, shorter girls were perceived as less competent than girls of average or tall height, whereas tall and average height girls were not viewed differently. Mothers tended to view the shorter girls as more dependent and helpless, but not less compliant or socially inept. Mothers rated the smaller girls as significantly younger than either girls of average or above average height. In addition, greater punishment was given to taller girls than to smaller girls, regardless of the perceived age of the child. Taken together, these two studies have implications for the expectations and reinforcements children might receive based on their stature. The authors interpret these results as demonstrative of cultural stereotypes such that smaller girls are considered more "feminine" (e.g. , dainty, defenseless, cute, dependent) and taller 15 boys are considered more "masculine" (e.g. , strong, competent, dominant). In a third study, Eisenberg et a1. (1984) investigated the relationship of preschool children’s height to their cognitive development and to peer evaluations of competency. Boys’ but not girls’ performance on Piagetian tasks of logic was positively associated with height, after controlling for age, but there were few relationships between height and peer ratings of competence. Taller boys were rated as smarter by girls, and smaller boys were rated as better at art projects by both boys and girls. There were no height effects for peer judgments of girls’ competence. In light of data suggesting that intelligence is unrelated to the stature of young children (Bayley, 1956), the authors conclude that taller boys’ better cognitive performance and greater popularity might be due to responses from their social environment, rather than superior innate abilities. They suggest that parents (and other adults) might be more active in shaping the cognitive development of boys than girls, and that this would be especially true for taller boys, who elicit expectations of greater competency. Brackbill and Nevill (1981) found similar results in their studies investigating the effect of children’s height on parental expectations of achievement. In their first study parents of 11 year-old children were shown two sets of stimulus pictures depicting male and female children of different heights, all presumed to be 11 years- old. Parents were asked to assign 9 chores to each of the 5 children (either all male or female). In spite of knowledge of the children’s chronological age, parents consistently had greater expectations of mastery and competence for the taller 16 children, regardless of gender. In the second study three groups of female adults, with varying degrees of experience with children, were shown two sets of pictures depicting two same—sexed children. In the first picture the children were the same height but were assigned different ages. In the second picture the children were different heights and the younger child was taller. Subjects assigned chores of varying difficulty to each of the children. Similar to the results in the first study, female adults assigned more difficult tasks to the taller children, regardless of age. Consistent with Eisenberg et al.’s (1984) results, these findings suggest a strong link between parental expectations for achievement and competence and children’s height. They also lend support to the notion that the degree of children’s achievement motivation may stem from the presence or lack of parental (and other adult) pressure to achieve (Brackbill & Nevill, 1981). P_sychosocial Adjustment to SS Psychosocial Outcomes in Childhood and Adolescence Cognitive Functioning and Academic Achievement: Recent investigations of children with GHD and CD consistently report high rates of grade retention and academic underachievement. In their investigation of grade retention and academic achievement in a mixed sample of children with SS (CD, Turner Syndrome [TS], and GHD), Holmes, Thompson, and Hayford (1984) found that 21% of their subjects had been retained at least one grade. In spite of average intelligence, the retained children demonstrated a constellation of difficulties including: lower l7 verbal and performance abilities, greater conduct- and personality-disordered problems, and continued poor academic achievement, despite grade retention. Etiology did not appear to have an effect on grade retention, even though this sample included girls with Turner Syndrome, who have well-documented nonverbal and visual-spatial deficits. Those most likely to be retained were adolescent girls and young boys. In a sample of adults with GHD, 41% of all subjects repeated at least one grade, 10% repeated more than one grade, and elementary school grades were the most commonly repeated (Clopper et al., 1986). 81% of those repeating a grade did so in primary school, and of this group 62% repeated kindergarten or first grade. These differences may reflect a growing awareness of and sensitivity to psychosocial issues confronting children with differences, and a trend not to retain a child solely because of physical immaturity or size. Although there has been some conflicting data regarding the cognitive and academic functioning of children and adolescents with SS, there is general agreement that academic underachievement is a common problem in this population. Three theories have been proposed to explain this high incidence of academic failure: 1) the low ability theory, 2) the cognitive deficit theory, and 3) the cognitive underfunctioning theory (Siegel, 1982). According to the low ability theory, low achievement is consistent with subnormal intellectual abilities which are secondary to endocrine dysfunction. The cognitive deficit theory holds that underachievement is related to specific cognitive, visual-spatial, and attentional deficits. Finally, the 18 cognitive underfunctioning theory proposes that discrepancies between intellectual abilities and academic achievement are secondary to psychosocial and environmental factors, such as low self-esteem resulting from SS and poor parenting (Siegel, 1990). Each of these theories has gained some degree of support from the existing body of literature. One of the most recent and comprehensive investigations of the intellectual and academic functioning of children with SS began about ten years ago through a collaborative effort of investigators throughout Michigan. In the first of these studies, Siegel (1982) examined the cognitive and affective functioning of 42 children with GHD. Two subgroups were included: 27 subjects experienced SS related to isolated growth hormone deficiency (IGHD), while 14 had SS secondary to multiple hormone deficiencies (MHD) which affected their growth hormone secretion and uptake. Subjects were between the ages of 6 and 16 years and were assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R). Overall intelligence fell within the low average range with a significantly higher incidence of verbal- performance differences than the normative sample. As a group, the GHD children had lower scores on the freedom from distractibility factor of the WISC-R. The freedom from distractibility factor is comprised of the arithmetic, digit span, and coding subscales, and low scores on this factor indicate difficulties with attention, sequencing, concentration, and short-term memory. Visual-spatial deficits and lower math achievement were also evident in 38 % of the sample. Despite these academic difficulties, these children did not rate themselves as having lower self-esteem than 19 the normative sample, and their mothers did not report behaviors indicating overprotectiveness. Subjects with MHD had developmental histories which were marked by significantly more delays and vulnerabilities than the IGHD group. Mothers of children with MHD were more overprotective than mothers of children with IGHD, perhaps in response to the perception of their children as more vulnerable. In a three-year follow-up study, Siegel and Hopwood (1986) found achievement problems in 52% of the children with GHD, with over 33% failing at least one grade in school. Learning problems were assessed with the reading and math subtests of the Wide Range Achievement Test and the reading comprehension section of the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test. Underachievers (those who demonstrated learning difficulties) were significantly more likely to have repeated a grade, and had significantly lower scores on measures of intellect, achievement, and visual-spatial skills than those without learning problems. By contrast, there were no differences between achievers and underachievers on self-esteem and maternal attitudes. With regard to the three aforementioned theories explaining underachievement, 40% of the underachieving children had profiles compatible with the low ability theory; 36% were consistent with the cognitive deficit theory; and the underfunctioning theory applied to 24%. In the context of average abilities, the children in the underfunctioning group did not have lower self-esteem or more protective mothers. Of note is the fact that as a group, the children with GHD rated themselves higher on self—concept than the normative sample (Siegel & Hopwood, 20 1986). The authors suggest that other factors not measured in the study may have accounted for the academic problems in the underachieving group, such as motivation, anxiety, or stress (Siegel & Hopwood, 1986). The third Michigan study was a seven-year follow-up of 28 of the original subjects (Siegel et al., 1988). As assessed by the WISC-R, overall intelligence for the children and adolescents with GHD remained stable (in the low average range), and scores were lower on the freedom from distractibility scale than the performance or verbal scales. Visual-spatial skills showed improvement and fell within the normal range, which was consistent with this group’s low average cognitive abilities. Improvement was also noted in subjects’ self-concept, particularly in the areas of physical attraction and popularity, however, academic underachievement problems persisted in about one third of the sample, with 74% of the original underachievers having continued difficulties. As a group, underachievers had lower freedom from distractibility scores than verbal or performance scores on the WISC-R. In addition, their parents rated them as having significant problems with immaturity and hyperactivity, characteristics associated with attention problems. These results are consistent with those of other researchers (Abbott et al., 1982; Lewis et al. , 1986; Ryan et a1. , 1988), and suggest that both attentional and cognitive deficits play an important role in the underachievement of children with GHD (Siegel, 1990). Compared to the extensive research on academic and intellectual functioning in children with GHD, fewer studies have investigated this area in children with CD and F88. Only one study includes both individuals with F88 and CD (Gold, 1978). 21 Results suggest that children with CD have greater learning problems, and are diagnosed with attentional and hyperactivity problems more frequently than their counterparts with FSS. In addition, half of the children with CD had been described as immature in school or medical records, while this was true for only 1 out of 435 children with FSS. Gordon and his colleagues (Gordon et al., 1982; Gordon et al., 1984) compared 24 children with CD to a group of healthy children, matched for gender, age, and SES. They found no differences between the groups on measures of academic, intellectual, or visual—motor functioning (Gordon et a1. , 1984). However, children with CD were rated by their parents as having significantly more somatic complaints, schizoidal tendencies, and social withdrawal (Gordon et al. , 1982). Parents of children with CD also reflected greater difficulties setting clear limits, though they did not appear to be overly protective. Families with short children were also described as having poorer cooperation and communication. Children with CD reported lower self-concept, reflected by greater unhappiness and feeling unpopular. In a review of the literature on the psychosocial correlates of short stature and delayed puberty, Lee and Rosenfeld (1987) conclude that overall, children with short stature have average intellectual abilities, in spite of their somewhat depressed scores on standardized tests. They also report that there is no conclusive evidence that short stature is associated with psychopathology or impaired school performance. In a more recent review article on the psychological impact of SS, Siegel, Clopper, & Stabler (1991) summarize: 22 "to date, the only clear conclusion is that many significantly short children have learning problems in school, and that a combination of cognitive, physiological and psychosocial factors appear to contribute. However, it has not yet been determined whether specific learning profiles are more likely to be associated with specific diagnostic classifications" (p. 15). Psychosocial Functioning and Social Competence: From the discussion above, it is apparent that no one conclusion can be drawn about the psychosocial functioning of children with short stature, as the data are fraught with inconsistencies. Whereas earlier investigations concluded that children with 88 suffered from impaired self-esteem, more recent studies indicate that children with SS differentiate between separate areas of their self-concept, and that all are not uniformly low (Young-Hyman, 1990). It has also been suggested that by devaluing physical appearance, children with SS are able to maintain adequate global self-worth, and to derive esteem from those areas in which they feel most competent (Clopper, 1992; Crocker & Major, 1989). In contrast to reports of children with SS feeling unhappy or unpopular (Gordon et a1. , 1982; Rotnem et a1. , 1977), many children and adolescents with 88 rate themselves as having high, somewhat elevated self-esteem (e.g., Young-Hyman, 1986). Furthermore, many children with SS are able to make accurate appraisals about their stature (Young-Hyman, 1986) and report satisfaction with their physical appearance (Gordon et al., 1982). 23 With regard to social competence, children with S8 have demonstrated a pattern of social withdrawal, immature behavior, and a preference for individual rather than group activities (Gordon et al., 1982; Holmes et al., 1982, 1984; Young- Hyman, 1986). Short children tend to have fewer, but more long-term close friendships (Young-Hyman, 1986). These results may be due in part to the short child’s fear of social situations in which s/he might encounter teasing, juvenilization, social ostracism, or negative feedback regarding performance or capabilities. According to Stabler and his colleagues (1980), "Not only does short stature limit the range of social roles available to the individual, but, in addition, short children tend to exhibit social behaviors which accentuate the discrepancy between their chronological age and physical stature " (p. 743). There is some evidence that children with SS have impaired social judgment, low tolerance for frustration, and decreased competitiveness and aggressiveness (Drotar et al., 1980; Holmes et al., 1982; Stabler et al., 1980; Steinhausen & Stahnke, 1976; Young-Hyman, 1986). For example, Young-Hyman (1986) found that children and adolescents with CD, FSS, and GHD scored significantly lower on a social problem-solving task than the normative sample. In spite of this difficulty, subjects rated themselves as more competent than age norms in social skills, self- esteem, and cognitive abilities. Along similar lines, Drotar et a1. (1980) studied frustration tolerance and problem-solving in children with SS. This sample of children with GHD had low tolerance for frustration, and poorer, less adaptive and mature problem-solving skills. 24 In their investigation of social judgments and competitiveness in children with SS, Stabler and his colleagues (1980) administered a test of cause and effect in social situations under competitive and noncompetitive conditions to a group of male children with SS and a group of age-matched male children with normal stature. While the children with SS demonstrated lower social judgment abilities than matched controls under both conditions, they did exhibit an increase in scores during the competitive trial. These results provide evidence that under conditions of increased competition, children with SS have appropriate levels of motivation to achieve. It can be argued that adolescence would be a period of particular challenge for the individual with SS, as delayed physical and sexual maturation would become more prominent in the context of peer attainment of puberty, and might thwart or retard the normative developmental processes of identity formation, individuation, socialization, and psychosexual mastery (Dean et al., 1986). Holmes et al.’s (1986) longitudinal study of the psychosocial correlates of SS lends support to the hypothesis of increased distress during adolescence. Subjects were children and adolescents who had been followed for three years after their initial evaluation for SS due to GHD, CD, or Turner Syndrome (TS). Results indicate that the subjects experienced an age-related decline in adjustment during adolescence, with decreases in school and social competence at 12 and 14 years of age. During this period psychosocial functioning fell to about 1 SD. below the mean, while it was preceded and followed by functioning at the 50th percentile. By approximately 17 years of age, school and social competence had normalized. The most significant academic problems were 25 experienced by adolescent females (primarily those with TS and GHD), whose performance was 1.5 SD. below the mean. By contrast, younger children with CD had the highest academic performance, and boys generally performed at their expected age levels. Overall, younger children with SS exhibited more behavior problems and demonstrated significantly more externalizing and internalizing behavior problems (e.g. , social withdrawal and somatic complaints). In their investigation of boys with CD and GHD, Holmes, Hayford, and Thompson (1982) found no differences in behavior patterns based on diagnosis. They did, however, find that age was a more important factor in psychological adjustment than specific diagnoses. Older boys with SS were rated by their parents as having significantly more obsessive-compulsive and less aggressive tendencies than younger boys, and older boys were more conforming and inhibited. Parents rated both age groups as significantly more withdrawn than the normative sample for the measure. Taken together, the data concerning the psychosocial adjustment of children and adolescents with SS depicts a fairly well-functioning group, with specific deficits in academic and social competence. Younger children appear to have greater behavior problems in general, while adolescence is characterized by more internalizing behavior problems and deficits in self-competence. While there is little empirical evidence suggesting significant differences in the psychosocial functioning of boys and girls, the literature on social stigma and stereotypes supports the hypothesis that girls with SS will experience fewer psychosocial difficulties. This is partially supported by the higher referral rates of boys to specialty clinics for 26 concerns about stature. Until the research in this area takes on a more biopsychosical and systemic approach, however, no firm conclusions can be drawn regarding the multiple factors which affect child adjustment to SS. As Siegel (1990) aptly summarizes the current state of affairs, "the literature to date is glaringly void of studies investigating family interactional patterns in terms of the differences that cohesiveness, adaptability, and triangulation may have on the overall functioning of these physically and psychologically vulnerable children" (p. 37). The present study represents an attempt to fill this gap in the existing body of literature by addressing the family factors and processes that impact children’s psychosocial adjustment to SS. Psychosocial Outcomes in Adulthood The long-term psychosocial outcomes associated with SS have been documented primarily in adult populations of individuals with GHD, many of whom had received growth hormone therapy as children and/ or adolescents. These data provide a somewhat disconcerting picture of adult psychosocial functioning in individuals with SS, and in particular those with GHD. Lower rates of employment and marriage are consistently reported in this population than in matched control groups or regional norms (Bjork, Jonsson, Westphal, & Levin, 1989; Clopper, MacGillivray, Mazur, Voorhess, & Mills, 1986; Dean, McTaggart, Fish, & Friesen, 1986; McGauley, 1989; Mitchell, Libber, Johanson, Plotnik, Joyce, Migeon, & Blizzard, 1986; Ranke, 1987). Of note is that rates of employment and marriage in at least one population of adults with GHD are lower than those found in populations of 27 adults with other chronic diseases (Dean, McTaggart, Fish, and Friesen, 1986). Furthermore, these relatively poor psychosocial outcomes appear to be unrelated to final adult height attained (Dean et al., 1986; Zimet et al., 1993). Many of these adults with GHD did not live independently from their parents and fewer obtained driver’s licenses than in comparable age groups (Bjork et al., 1989; Clopper et a1. , 1986; Dean et al. , 1986) The majority of these subjects report that juvenilization continued to impact their lives negatively both professionally and socially in adulthood (Clopper et al., 1986; Dean et al., 1986; Mitchell et al., 1986). Adults with SS also have reported lower quality of life, greater social isolation, greater health problems, more anxiety and depression, higher rates of participation in counselling, and lower feelings of well-being than adults of normal stature (Bjork et al., 1989; Dean eta 1., 1986; McGauley, 1989; Mitchell et al., 1986). By contrast, in Mitchell et al.’s (1986) study of adults with GHD, subjects rated themselves as higher on self—satisfaction, personal worth, and sociability, and lower on physical attributes and self-criticism than a comparison group. The authors interpret these results as indicating a significant degree of denial and repression in their sample of adults with GHD (Mitchell et al., 1986). Zimet and his colleagues (1993) also report higher rates of restraint and repressive defensiveness in his sample of short adults without GHD than in a normative sample. Finally, despite an improvement in height, those individuals who received GH therapy were still significantly short as adults and continued to manifest numerous psychosocial difficulties (e.g., Dean et al., 1986; Bjork et al., 1989). On the other hand, many 28 researchers argue that compared to adults of normal stature, those with SS do not demonstrate significantly more psychosocial distress and live productive and fulfilling lives (e.g., Clopper et al., 1986; Mitchell et al., 1986; Zimet et al., 1993). Family Factors and Child Adjustment to SS Conflict, Control, and Support: There is evidence that having a child with medical concerns results in the perception of the child as "vulnerable" (Green & Solnit, 1964; Thomasgard & Metz, 1995). This process has also been reported in parents of children with SS (Rotnem et al. , 1980) and may exacerbate parents’ existing concerns about a child who is physically challenged and different from his/her peers. This view of the child with SS as vulnerable or at risk can result in parent-child interactions which promote juvenilization, overprotection, difficulty setting appropriate limits, overconcerns with bodily functioning, and academic underachievement (Clopper et al. , 1986; Rotnem et al., 1977; Young-Hyman, 1986). The psychosocial competence of children with SS can be fostered, however, within a nurturing and supportive family environment in which parents encourage age- appropriate behavior and development (Abbott et a1. , 1982; Rotnem et al., 1977; Young-Hyman, 1986). The dimensions of family support, control, and conflict have long been implicated as important factors impacting children’s psychosocial adjustment in general, and self-esteem in particular. For example, Hoelter & Harper (1987) examined a number of family variables (family size, composition, conflict, and 29 support) and their relationship to adolescent self-esteem. These authors found that family support was the most important family factor in the development of adolescent self-esteem and identity salience for both boys and girls. In addition, family conflict was negatively related to adolescents’ self-esteem. Parental authoritarianism and control also have also been linked to lower self—esteem in adolescents (Buri et al., 1988; Buri, 1989; Demo et al., 1987), while authoritative parenting is positively related to adolescents’ self-esteem (Buri, 1989). Children’s perceptions of family support and cohesiveness are positively related to their self-esteem, while their perceptions of family conflict are inversely correlated with self-esteem (Cooper et al., 1983). Further, adolescents’ perceptions of good communication and participation with their parents is associated with higher self-esteem. Gecas and Schwalbe (1986) found gender differences in the relationship between parent behaviors (support, control/ autonomy, and participation) and adolescents’ self-perceptions. Boys’ self-esteem was more affected by the control/autonomy dimension, while girls’ self-esteem was more dependent on parental participation and support. Fathers’ behaviors were more consequential than mothers’ for the development of self-esteem, especially for boys. Of note is that children’s perceptions of the family environment, rather than the parents’ , are more strongly associated with children’s reports of self-esteem (Demo et al., 1987; Gecas & Schwalbe, 1986). The family factors of support, control, and conflict have also been linked to children’s behavioral, social, and academic competence. There is a particularly large 30 body of literature linking family conflict, especially conflict between parents, to child behavior problems (see Block et al., 1981, 1986; Christensen et al., 1983). The relationship between parental conflict and child behavior problems has been stronger for boys than for girls (Emery & O’Leary, 1982; Block et al., 1981, 1986). Parental discord has also been related to children’s feelings of unacceptance (Emery & O’Leary, 1982). In adolescent populations, family conflict has been associated with poorer psychological adjustment, increased anxiety, lower self-esteem and life satisfaction, and lower feelings of control (Enos & Handal, 1986; Slater & Haber, 1984). In addition, Moos and Moos (1986) report that a supportive and stimulating family environment is most conducive to children’s social and cognitive development. Further, they report that families characterized by structure and support foster children’s scholastic self-concepts, while highly controlling and conflicted families do not. Finally, in a group of children with poor school performance, greater behavior problems (particularly internalizing behaviors) were found in children whose families were less supportive and more controlling (Thompson et a]. , 1990). Family functioning has also been strongly associated with both child and parent adjustment to childhood chronic illnesses. According to Moos and Moos (1986), "In general, supportive family environments, characterized by high cohesion and expressiveness and low conflict, are associated with family members’ better adjustment and greater ability to deal with stress, especially when coping with personal physical illness or a spouse’s mental and behavior problems, " (p. 30). Similarly, Thompson and his colleagues (1992a) found that mothers of children with 31 cystic fibrosis demonstrated higher levels of adjustment when their families were supportive and less conflicted. Kronenberger and Thompson (1990) derived three factors from the Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos, 1986) in a sample of families with chronically ill children: supportive, conflicted, and controlling factors. Chronically ill children whose families were supportive had fewer internalizing behavior problems and higher scores on sociability. In addition, children from conflicted families had more externalizing behavior problems and lower scores on sociability. The results underscore the family’s ability to moderate the effects of stress on the functioning of children and their families. In a sample of adolescents with spina bifida, Murch and Cohen (1989) found that low family conflict and control buffered the effects of stressful life events on the adolescents’ psychological adjustment. Family conflict has been associated with externalizing behavior problems in a group of children with five different chronic illnesses (W allander et al. , 1989). On the other hand, family cohesion and expressiveness were positively related to social competence, while control was negatively related to social competence. These "psychological family resources" accounted for a significant amount of the variance in the chronically ill children’s psychological and social adjustment, beyond that accounted for by "family utilitarian resources" (i.e. , maternal education and family income). Family communication and ability to resolve conflict have also been related to adolescents’ adjustment to diabetes (W ysocki, 1993). Along similar lines, patients with psychosomatic disorders tend to come from families which are relatively high on 32 control (Waring & Russell, 1980). These results are consistent with the clinical observations of Minuchin and his colleagues (i.e., Minuchin, Rosman, & Baker, 1978). In sum, this body of research indicates that the family environment has important effects on child self-esteem, and academic and psychosocial competence. These results are consistent across families with healthy children and children with chronic illness. In particular, families that are characterized by mutual support, authoritative parental control which encourages age-appropriate child mastery, and low levels of conflict, buffers the stresses associated with child chronic illness in both parents and children. Within this family environment, optimal levels of parental and child adjustment are possible, in spite of the many challenges imposed by child chronic illness. These family factors of support, control, and conflict, are deemed equally important for the psychosocial adaptation of children with SS. Short stature is comparable to other childhood chronic illnesses in many ways: its chronicity, the somewhat uncontrollable nature of its course, the need for long-term medical interventions and sometimes invasive procedures, the emotional and financial stresses it places on the child and family, parental concerns about treatment decisions and the child’s physiological and psychosocial development, and its constant threat to the child’s sense of mastery, independence, and control. Therefore, it is hypothesized that high levels of family support, low levels of control, and low levels of conflict will predict higher levels of child self-esteem and psychosocial competence in children with SS. 33 Parental Self-Esteem and Impact of Stature: The relationships between a) parental self-esteem and child adjustment, and b) parents’ stature experiences and child adjustment, have only been examined in a peripheral manner. In their study of the lineage transmission of interpersonal competence, Filsinger and Lamke (1983) found strong relationships between parents’ social competence and their older adolescent’s interpersonal competence and social self-esteem. In addition, children’s success in intimate relationships was significantly inversely linked to mothers’ social distress and avoidance. Children’s feelings of social competence were significantly related to fathers’ social self-esteem. From these limited results we can predict that higher parental self-competence will be directly related to child self-esteem, and indirectly related to child psychosocial adjustment, via its effects on child self-esteem. Even less is known about the relationship of parental experiences related to stature and its impact on child functioning. One study found that parents who themselves were short viewed their children with SS as more socially competent than parents of average stature (Young-Hyman, 1986). This finding may suggest a compensatory process whereby parents with a history of SS themselves view their child with SS in a more positive light. It is likely, however, that parents’ experiences related to stature will have both positive and negative effects on child self-esteem, coping, and adjustment. This will largely be determined by the degree to which parents themselves have adjusted to their own experiences related to stature. Coping Strategies: There are no known studies which directly investigate the 34 coping styles of individuals with SS. In addition, only one study was found that assesses the relationship between parents’ and children’s defense and coping mechanisms. No significant relationships were found between the defenses and coping styles of male children and their fathers, and only moderate associations were found between mothers’ and sons’ coping styles (Peshkess, 1977). Furthermore, the description and assessment of children’s coping strategies has only recently begun to be investigated and there are few standardized instruments available to measure coping in children (Causey & Dubow, 1992; Knapp et al. , 1991). Coping has been defined as "constantly changing cognitive and behavioral efforts to manage specific external and/ or internal demands that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources of the person" (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p. 141). Coping strategies differ as a function of the perceived stress at hand. Problem- focused coping involves an individual’s attempts to mange or alter a stressor. Emotion-focused coping represents the individual’s efforts to regulate emotional distress caused by a stressor. Another taxonomy has been proposed which divides these mechanisms into approach and avoidance coping (Roth & Cohen, 1986). Approach coping strategies are "behavioral, cognitive, or emotional activities oriented toward a stressor (e. g. , seeking information), " while avoidance coping strategies are "behavioral, cognitive, or emotional activities oriented away from a stressor in order to avoid it (e.g., ignoring the stressor)," (Causey & Dubow, 1992, p. 47). In general, avoidance or emotion-focused coping strategies are associated with poorer adjustment, whereas approach or problem-focused coping techniques are 35 related to more positive functioning (Causey & Dubow, 1992). Using self-report and peer-report measures of coping with 4th through 6th graders, Causey and Dubow (1992) found that girls used approach strategies more often, while boys chose avoidance strategies more frequently. Approach coping was positively associated with behavioral conduct esteem and global self-worth. Avoidance coping was positively related to self-reported anxiety, and inversely related to global self-worth and behavioral conduct. Similarly, Compas et al. (1988) found that in children’s coping with an interpersonal stressor, emotion—focused coping was associated with greater self-reports and maternal reports of emotional and behavioral problems, while problem-focused coping was negatively related to adjustment difficulties. Avoidance coping strategies were used more often by adolescents with depressive or conduct disorders than by a healthy control or physically ill control group (Ebata & Moos, 1989). Studies of parental coping techniques reveal findings which are consistent with studies of children’s coping reported above. Problem-focused (approach) coping was associated with decreased psychological distress in mothers of physically disabled children, whereas emotion-focused (avoidance) coping was related to higher levels of maternal distress (Miller et al., 1992). Similarly, palliative (avoidance) coping was associated with poorer adjustment in mothers of children with cystic fibrosis (Thompson et al. , 1992a). Given the above evidence linking coping strategies to psychosocial adjustment, it is predicted that parents’ and children’s use of approach coping strategies will be 36 related to better child psychosocial outcomes, and reliance on avoidance coping strategies will be linked to poorer child adjustment. Transactional Stress and Coping Model Thompson and his colleagues (1992b) have proposed a transactional stress and coping model for chronic childhood illness which is based on an ecological systems theory perspective. Within this model chronic childhood illness represents a stressor to which the individual and family systems attempt to adapt. The relationship between illness and outcomes is a function of biomedical, psychosocial, and developmental processes (Thompson et al. , 1992b). Severity is the illness parameter, and age, gender, and SES are the demographic parameters. The focal point of the model is child and family processes which mediate the relationship between illness and outcomes, beyond the contributions of illness and demographic parameters. The model also includes psychosocial/mediational processes which potentially reduce the impact of stress. These include: cognitive appraisals of stress, expectations of locus of control and efficacy, coping methods, and family functioning. This model was used to investigate the processes in the emotional adjustment of mothers who had children with cystic fibrosis (Thompson, Gustafson, & Spock, 1992a). Maternal adjustment was not related to the illness severity or demographic parameters. Adjustment was, however, significantly related to the proposed psychosocial/mediational processes which accounted for 35 and 40% of the variance in maternal anxiety and depression, respectively (Thompson et al. , 1992a). Higher 37 maternal adjustment was associated with family supportiveness, low levels of perceived daily stress, and less reliance on palliative (avoidance) coping methods. This research model was subsequently expanded to include the processes which affect the psychological adjustment of children with chronic illness. These psychological/mediational processes include expectations of locus of control and self- esteem. Child coping methods are also included in the model, but have not been empirically tested, for lack of an adequate measure of children’s coping responses (Thompson et al. , 1992b). Based on an ecological-systems theory perspective, the authors predicted that child psychological adjustment would be affected by the stress and symptoms in other family members. This is based on a large body of literature linking parental depression with child emotional and behavior problems (e.g. , Compas et al., 1989; Daniels et al., 1987). The results supported the psychosocial mediational processes of child self-esteem and maternal anxiety in child adjustment. In particular, children who reported lower self-esteem also had poorer adjustment, as measured by maternal reports of behavior problems and self-reported distress. The illness and demographic parameters accounted for only small amounts of variance in child adjustment, whereas self-esteem accounted for a significant amount of the variance in child adjustment as reported by both mother and child. Maternal anxiety also accounted for a significant increase in variance in both mother— and child- reported child adjustment. These results highlight the interactional nature of parent and child psychological/mediational processes and their impact on child adjustment to chronic illness. 38 The present study tested a modified version of the Transactional Stress and Coping Model of Thompson and his colleagues (1992b) (see Figure 1). Changes have been made in order to accommodate the variables of interest in the present study which are thought to be related to children’s psychosocial adaptation to SS. Both parent and child coping were included in the model, and were assessed with semi- structured interviews. 39 088%an cause"? 353.“an 3.8333— 0883an Boom Seam: mi EEO A III Beam me 83:: 3:5 3300 533.2% 8868.5 iguana: 2.30 A llll 038m he 33:: Bunyan 85m wEmeU 3:83 £83.28 355 BaneUJoEaenvfiogsm Seam 8368.?” 382:5: Ea“..— ecafim team com 332 wfiqou can amen—m mmm om< ease magmas 8825.5.“ ofinauueEeEozmefiuE a 2:5 40 Limitations of Previous Research on SS The body of literature investigating the psychosocial and cognitive functioning of children with SS is marked by numerous inconsistencies. Differences in the results of comparable studies can be accounted for in part by methodological differences and shortcomings. For example, one of the major limitations of the literature on SS has been the practice of combining heterogeneous samples of children with SS, some of whom have well-documented cognitive deficits and physical stigmata (e.g. Turner Syndrome and achondroplasia). The inclusion of such disparate groups makes it difficult to isolate the effects of SS on psychosocial adjustment from the effects of other physical differences or cognitive and behavioral difficulties. In addition, combining children whose SS is secondary to cranial tumors or radiation for life- threatening illnesses with children with CD, GHD, and FSS, seems equally inappropriate. The power of statistical comparisons has been limited by small sample sizes, some of which do not exceed 15. In addition, few studies provide carefully matched control groups of children with normal stature. Researchers have not routinely controlled for socioeconomic status, which is especially important when studying cognitive and academic functioning. With regard to measurement issues, earlier studies on the psychosocial functioning of children with SS suffered from a lack of standardized and well- validated test instruments. Many of these investigations relied on anecdotal data. In addition, there has been a lack of consistency in the definitions of the operational constructs studied (Siegel, 1990). The majority of studies in this area have relied on 41 parent—, teacher-, or self-report measures. While these are all valuable sources of information which provide different perspectives, they are subject to defensive or otherwise biased responding. As discussed above, it is clear that denial and repression are common in both children and adults with SS (Siegel & Hopwood, 1986; Voss et al., 1993; Zimet, 1993), which brings into question the reliability of the subjective measures curently in use. Qualitative research methods (e.g. , semi- structured interviews) might allow for more in-depth querying and clarification when denial or minimization is thought to occur. Interview data could corroborate information obtained in self—report measures. By far the most serious shortcoming of this body of research is the failure to address family factors which might impact children’s psychosocial adjustment to SS. There is a clear need for a more systemic focus, which includes both parent and child perceptions and experiences, and addresses the relationships between these variables. Those studies that do include family factors are usually based on maternal reports only, with relative neglect of fathers’ contributions to children’s developmental outcomes. Fathers have been shown to be especially important to children’s developing self-concept (Amato & Ochiltree, 1986), particularly for male children (Gecas & Schwalbe, 1986). In addition, as Rotnem et al. (1982) found that fathers had greater difficulty accepting the diagnosis of SS than mothers, it is likely that their responses will have important implications for child adjustment to SS. Finally, there have been no investigations which look at the relationship between parental functioning (e.g. , self-esteem, coping strategies) and child adjustment to SS. This 42 may be especially important when parents themselves have a history of SS. Overview and Research Questions The present study was designed to improve upon past methodological weaknesses in the literature on child psychosocial adjustment to SS as follows: 1) While the current sample of children includes mixed diagnoses (CD, GHD, FSS, and CD/FSS), it does not include diagnostic categories in which affected individuals have additional cognitive, physical, or psychological difficulties (e.g. , TS, achondroplasia). 2) The psychosocial adjustment of children with SS was compared to that of a group of children with normal stature, matched for age, gender, ethnicity, and SES. 3) A major focus of this study was to test a stress and coping model for SS, including family and child psychological and mediational processes which were predicted to affect child outcomes. 4) Both mothers and fathers were included in this study. 5) Questionnaires were combined with a semi-structured interview in order to obtain more detailed qualitative data on the impact of stature and coping styles of parents and children. The following research questions were addressed in this investigation: 1) Do children with short stature demonstrate significantly more psychosocial and behavioral problems than a matched group of children of with normal stature? 2) Are there gender, age, or diagnosis effects in children’s adaptation to short 43 stature? 3) Do children with short stature devalue physical appearance as a way of maintaining overall high self-esteem and positive affect? 4) Do the family environments of children with short stature differ from those of children with normal stature? 5) How do family factors impact children’s adjustment to short stature? 6) How do the coping styles of parents relate to child coping and child adjustment? 7) Is a model of stress and coping useful for understanding the individual and family processes which affect child adjustment to SS? CHAPTER 2 Hypotheses 1) Children and adolescents with SS will be rated by their parents as having significantly more psychosocial and behavioral problems than a matched group of children with normal stature. 2) Children and adolescents with SS will rate themselves as having lower self- competence than a matched group of children with normal stature. 3) Boys with SS will demonstrate greater psychosocial and behavioral problems and lower self-competence than a) girls with SS, and b) children of normal stature. 4) Younger children with SS (under 12 years) will demonstrate a) greater self- competence, and b) greater psychosocial and behavior problems than older children and adolescents with SS (12 years and above). 5) Children with SS who have a perceived or diagnosed family history of SS (F88 and FSS/CD) will demonstrate better psychosocial adjustment than those without a perceived or diagnosed family history of SS (GHD and CD). 6) Children with SS who devalue physical appearance will have a) higher self- esteem, and b) fewer internalizing behavior problems than those who regard appearance as more important. 7) Families of children with SS will be more controlling than families of children with normal stature. 8) Families factors of a) high support, and b) low control and conflict will be 45 associated with: a) higher child self-esteem, and b) better adjustment (social, academic, and behavioral competence) both for a) children with SS, and b) children with normal stature. 9) The type of coping strategies used by parents will be positively related to the type of coping strategies used by their children. 10) Children’s approach (rather than avoidance) coping strategies will be associated with better child psychosocial adjustment. 11) The relationship between children’s psychosocial adjustment to SS and diagnostic/demographic parameters will be mediated by family emotional resources and mediational processes ( a) support, b) conflict, c) control, d) parents’ self-worth, e) parents’ impact of stature, and 0 parents’ coping strategies), and child mediational processes ( a) self—worth, b) impact of stature, and c) coping strategies). CHAPTER 3 Method Subjects Subjects were 33 children between the ages of 6 and 18 years with short stature (SS) and their parents, and a comparison group of 33 children of normal stature. There were 23 boys and 10 girls in each group. Participants were recruited through a university pediatric endocrinology clinic and were at or below the fifth percentile of height (-1.6 SD) expected for age and gender. Patients with a history of serious chronic illness, cranial tumor, developmental delay/mental retardation, achondroplasia, Turner’s Syndrome, chromosomal abnormalities, or physically disfiguring conditions were excluded from the study. These exclusion criteria were designed to eliminate other factors which might negatively impact the child and family’s adjustment to SS. In this manner, the subject population was limited to children and families whose primary challenge was coping with short stature and its attendant difficulties. Also excluded were patients who did not speak fluent English. Eligible participants carried the following diagnoses: 1) hypopituitarism/ growth hormone deficiency (GHD), including cases of growth hormone deficiency which are idiopathic or due to pituitary pathology (n=4); 2) familial short stature (FSS), in which the rate of growth is normal and short stature is linked to genetic inheritance (n=8); and 3) constitutional delay of growth (CD), in which a delayed rate of growth causes short stature and delayed puberty, but a normal final adult height is obtained (11: 16). Four subjects were diagnosed as having both familial short stature and 46 constitutional delay (FSS/CD). One subject had SS of unknown etiology. The following diagnostic criteria are used in the Michigan State University Pediatric Endocrinology Clinic where subjects with SS were recruited: 1.) GHD: a) peak GH of < 10 ng/ml after administration of a provocative stimulus and/or; b) 24-hr. average concentration of GH < 3.2 ng/ml and; c) bone age delay > 2 SD’s from chronological age and; (1) growth rate < 4.2 cm/yr. 2.) CD: a) bone age delay > 2 SD’s from chronological age and; b) 5th percentile height or below. 3.) FSS: a) 5th percentile height or below b) bone age within 2 SD’s of age expectancy. Subjects who have received or were receiving some type of pharmacological intervention (e. g. , GH) were not excluded from the study, as long as they met the criterion for height (at or below the 5th %tile) at the time of recruitment. Twenty- four subjects had never received pharmacological treatment; 3 had in the past but were not currently; and 6 were currently under treatment (2 for under 6 months and 4 for over six months) at the time of their participation. Of the 39 potential candidates approached to participate in the study, four families declined. Three of these families were concerned about possible negative repercussions of participation and did not want to emphasize their childrens’ SS. The fourth family was in the process of a divorce. Although the parents were willing to 47 48 participate, the child declined. Two more families agreed to participate, but did not respond to attempts to contact them after the initial meeting. In three additional families the children agreed to complete questionnaires, but did not wish to participate in the interview. In one family, the mother completed questionnaires, but deferred participation in the interview. Thirty children with SS and their families provided both questionnaire and interview data, while 3 completed only questionnaire data. The 33 subjects with SS came from predominantly middle class, Caucasian, two-parent families. Socioeconomic status was measured by the Revised Duncan Standardized Socioeconomic Index (TSEIZ) averaging both parents’ scores, as recommended by Mueller and Parcel (1981). The mean TSEIZ of these families was 40.22 (consistent with managerial and administrative positions) with a range of 17.70 to 76.90 (laborers to professionals). The average yearly income of this group fell in the $40,000-$49,000 range. The mean age of the children was 11.91 years, with a range of 6.25 to 17.83 years. Twenty-eight of the children were Caucasian, 2 were Asian, 1 was Hispanic, and 1 was bi—racial (Caucasian/African-American). Three of the children were adopted as infants. A control group of 33 children of normal stature (between the 10th and 90th percentiles of height for age and gender) who were never referred to a physician because of concerns related to stature, were matched with the SS subjects on gender, age (+/- 11 months), SES, and ethnicity. These subjects were recruited through a primary care pediatric clinic and through flyers circulated within the same geographic area from which the SS group was drawn (e.g. , at schools, churches, and through 49 personal contacts). The same exclusion criteria discussed above were used in selecting matched controls. The mean age of this group was 11.65 years with a range of 5.83 to 17.33 years. Adequate matches were made for ethnicity for all subjects except one Hispanic girl with SS who was matched with a Caucasian girl of normal stature. The mean Duncan TSEI2 for the comparison group was 42.80 and was not significantly different from that of the SS group (t= .59; p= .55). One of the children in this group was adopted as an infant. Procedures Potential SS subjects were identified from a patient roster prior to their appointment at the pediatric endocrinology clinic. They were sent a letter explaining the purpose of the study and were informed that they would be called within one week in order to answer any questions and to discuss their interest in participation (see Appendix B). If the parent(s) and the child agreed to participate, a meeting with the primary investigator was arranged either before or after their clinic appointment to discuss issues pertaining to confidentiality and consent, and to administer questionnaires and conduct an interview with each family member individually. Families were assured that confidentiality would be maintained, that their participation in the study would not impact their medical care in any way, that the information they provided throughout the course of the study would not be part of their medical record, and that they could terminate participation at any time. These procedures to ensure participant confidentiality and ethical treatment have been approved by the University 50 Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) (see Appendices C and D). Both written and verbal instructions were given for completing the questionnaires (see Appendix E). Parents and children were instructed to complete their forms independently and not to discuss their answers until everyone had completed them. If a child required assistance when the investigator was unavailable, parents were requested to help their child without influencing responses. In order to reduce the amount of time required to complete both the questionnaires and interviews, while one family member completed the interview, the others filled out their questionnaires. The order of administration of the interview and questionnaires was randomly alternated so that an equal number of parents and children completed the interview prior to the questionnaires. All interviews were conducted privately and were videotaped. In cases where families were not comfortable being videotaped, the interviews were audiotaped. In some instances, it was not possible to send letters to potential subjects with SS prior to their appointment at the endocrine clinic because of late additions to the clinic schedule, schedule or address changes, or uncertainty about diagnoses. Recruitment then occured during their clinic visit and the interviews were scheduled at a convenient time for the family. The comparison group of children with normal stature (NS) was recruited with flyers in a primary care pediatrics clinic and other community establishments (e.g. , churches, sports facilities, childcare centers, and laundromats), and through personal contacts. Families were screened briefly for exclusionary criteria and for 51 demographic variables, to ensure their appropriateness and match. All other procedures were the same as those followed with the SS subjects. While it would have been preferable to conduct all of the interviews in person, some of the families traveled a great distance to clinic (e.g., over 350 miles) and/or were unable to stay for the interviews after their appointment, or to return to or meet at the Clinical Center. Under these circumstances, the primary investigator either conducted the interviews in the subject’s home or over the telephone. When face-to- face interviews were not possible, telephone interviews were deemed appropriate and practical, and they prevented the loss of subjects due to geographic or time constraints. All phone interviews were audiotaped and families completed their questionnaires independently at home and returned them in the mail. A considerable body of literature comparing face-to-face with telephone interviews lends support to mixing these two methodological approaches. For example, studies investigating adult hypertension (Chwalow, Balkav, Costigliola, & Deeds, 1989), psychiatric epidemiology (Fenig, Levav, Kohn, & Yelin, 1993), depression (Wells, Burnam, Leake, & Robins, 1988), drug use and sexual behavior in Hispanics (Marin & Marin, 1989), and sexual behavior in college females (Berrnak, E. , 1989) have found significant agreement between the two methods with no significant differences with regards to rate and depth of self-disclosure ( e. g., Quinn, Gutek, & Walsh, 1980), interviewee discomfort, codability, and accuracy of response. Chwalow et al. (1989) state that "the use of more than one method of data collection with the same sample, while facilitating the augmentation of the response rate, will 52 not necessarily bias the study results," (p.321). In addition, Rogers (1976) concludes that "the quality of data obtained by telephone on complex attitudinal and knowledge items, as well as personal items is comparable to that collected in person," (p. 51). While this body of data is based on adult subjects, Reich & Earls (1990) conducted a parallel study with an adolescent population (12-17 yrs.). They found that face-to- face and telephone interviewing did not differ in the reporting of diagnostic categories or in response rates to embarrassing or personal questions. Adolescent subjects interviewed by telephone did, however, report fewer symptoms than those interviewed in person. There have been no known comparable investigations involving telephone and face-to-face interviews with children. A greater proportion of telephone interviews were conducted for the comparison group than the SS group. In the SS group, 37% of the children, 38% of the mothers, and 65 % of the fathers participated in telephone interviews. In the comparison group, 53% of the children, 53% of the mothers, and 81% of the fathers were interviewed by telephone. This difference was due in part to fewer opportunities for personal contact with the comparison families, and possibly less investment on their part in study participation. Measures Demographic Parameters/ Instrumental Family Resources Wallander and his colleagues (1989) have defined utilitarian family resources as "structural characteristics of the family which may aid in dealing with acute and 53 chronic problems related to the child’s handicap" (p. 376). Previous research has included the following characteristics as utilitarian or instrumental family resources: parents’ age and education, family income, family size, and duration of marriage. In the present study, each parent completed a Child and Family Information Questionnaire in order to provide information regarding family demographics as well as instrumental family resources such as: parents’ ethnicity, education, employment status, family size, and gross annual income (see Appendix F). Family Mediational Processes Psychosocial Family Resources: The psychosocial family resources of interest in the present study include family support, control, and conflict. These three factors were assessed with the Family Environment Scale-2nd Edition (FES-Moos & Moos, 1986) and the Children’s Version of the Family Environment Scale (CVFES- Pino, Simons, & Slawinowski, 1984). The FES is a 90-item true-false questionnaire, comprised of 10 subscales, designed for adolescents and adults. Children from 6 to 12 years completed the CVFES, which pictorially depicts 30 items comparable to those on the PBS, and yields the same 10 subscales. The items on the CVFES require children to choose which of three pictures most closely resembles their own family. The 10 subscales of the PBS and CVFES assess environmental characteristics of the family within three broad domains: the Personal Growth Domain consists of types of family activities, interests, and values, and includes the Achievement Orientation, Independence, Active-Recreational Orientation, Moral-Religious, and 54 Intellectual-Cultural subscales; the System Maintenance Domain represents the degree to which structure, control, and rules govern family life and is composed of the Control and Organization subscales; finally, the Relationship Domain characterizes the importance and types of interpersonal relationships within the family and includes the Expressiveness, Cohesion, and Conflict subscales. Moos and Moos (1986) report internal consistencies ranging from .61 to .78 and two-month test-retest reliabilities ranging from .68 to .86 for the PBS subscales. The authors’ findings also support adequate construct and discrmininant validity of the subscales (Moos & Moos, 1986). In their work investigating characteristics of families with chronically ill and healthy children, Kroneberger and Thompson (1990) derived three higher order dimensions characterizing family environments: the Supportive, Controlling, and Conflicted factors. The Supportive factor represents "the degree of mutual commitment and support for expression of feelings and for active participation in social and recreational activities" (Thompson, et al., 1992a, p. 579) and is composed of the sum of scaled scores from the Cohesion, Expressiveness, Active-Recreational Orientation, Intellectual-Cultural Orientation, and Independence subscales (Chronbach alpha = .81). The Controlling factor reflects "emphasis upon control, ethical and religious values, achievement orientation, and a lack of independence" (Thompson et al., 1992a, p. 579) and is the sum of the Control, Moral-Religious Emphasis, and Achievement Orientation subscale scores, minus the score for the Independence subscale (Chronbach alpha = .65). The Conflicted factor represents "high conflict, poor organization, and a lack of mutual commitment and support" within the family 55 (Thompson et al., 1992a, p. 579) and is comprised of the Conflict score minus the scores for the Cohesion and Organization scales (Chronbach alpha = .84). Kronenberger and Thompson (1990) demonstrated the construct validity and utility of these three higher order factors in their investigation of family environments of chronically ill and healthy children. They found that chronically ill children with behavior problems came from more conflicted and less supportive families. More specifically, the Conflicted factor was positively related to externalizing behavior problems and negatively related to sociability. The Supportive factor was negatively related to Internalizing behavior problems and positively associated with sociability. These investigators also replicated the three factors in a sample of healthy children. Additional studies have linked the three factors to child psychosocial adjustment. For example, Thompson, Lampron, Johnson & Eckstein (1990) found that in a sample of children with poor academic performance, children with greater behavior problems (especially internalizing behaviors) had less supportive and more conflicted families. In addition, in a sample of children with myelodysplasia, the Supportive and Conflicted dimensions were related to internalizing and externalizing behavior problems, respectively (Thompson, Kronenberger, Johnson, & Whiting, 1989). Based on Moos and Moos’ (1986) construct of the family environment profile, family members’ individual scores for each of the three factors (hereafter referred to as support, control, and conflict) were averaged into composite family scores in the present study. This approach was consistent with the view of the family as a system, and reflected the transactional nature of family members’ perceptions and behaviors. 56 It also facilitated the examination of the relationship between the family climate and child psychosocial outcomes. Parental Self-Worth: Parents’ feelings of self-competence was assessed with the Self-Perception Profile for Adults (Messer & Harter, 1986). This 50—item questionnaire taps the following twelve domains: intelligence, job competence, athletic competence, physical appearance, sociability, close friendship, intimate relationships, morality, sense of humor, nurturance, household management, adequacy as a provider, and global self-worth. This questionnaire has a similar format to the Self-Perception Profiles for Children and Adolescents (see below). Messer and Harter (1986) report internal consistency reliabilities ranging from .63 to .92 for two different samples of adults including full-time working men and women, part-time working women, and homemakers. The adequate provider scale was found to be unreliable in samples of homemakers and part-time working women. Impact of Stature and Coping Strategies: The impact of stature on the parents and their coping strategies were also assessed as additional mediational processes to adequate child adjustment. The Semi-Structured Interview detailed below provided the data for these variables. Child Mediational Processes Self-Worth: Harter’s Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter, 1982) and Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter, 1988) were used to assess subject’s self-perceptions of their own competencies in a number of different domains: 57 academic, social, athletic, physical appearance, and behavioral conduct. The adolescent profile also includes three additional scales which were not used in this study: job competence, close friendship, and romantic appeal. Both child and adolescent forms also include a global self—worth scale. The children’s form includes 36 items, while the adolescent form contains 45. Each item depicts two contrasting statements, from which children choose the one they are most like (e.g. , "Some kids are happy with their height and weight BUT Other kids wish their height or weight were different. "). Second, they decide if the statement is " really true" or "sort of true" for them. The alpha reliabilities of the scale for children range from .73 to .87 (Harter, 1982; 1983) and for adolescents they range from .85 to .95 (Harter, 1988). For the Self-Perception Profile for Children, Harter (1983) reports good test-retest reliabilities for samples retested at three month and nine month intervals and adequate convergent and construct validity. Particularly relevant to this study is the finding that in a sample of 6th and 7th graders, physical appearance was the domain most highly correlated with global self- worth. Also of note were moderate relations among the social acceptance, athletic competence, and physical appearance domains. Harter (1983) speculates that athletic ability and physical attractiveness lead to greater popularity and acceptance among peers. Of special interest to this study is the degree to which subjects value physical appearance. In order to assess the degree to which physical appearance is valued (or devalued), subjects completed an importance rating for this domain. Importance 58 ratings are included at the end of the Self-Perception Profile for Children and Adolescents. Items on the Importance Scale are structured similarly to those on the Self-Perception Profiles. First, subjects choose which of two statements is most like them. Second, they indicate whether this is "sort of true" or "really true" for them (e.g., "Some kids think it’s important to be good looking in order to feel good about themselves BUT Other kids don’t think that’s very important at all"). Impact of Stature and Coping Strategies: The impact of stature on the children and their coping strategies were also assessed as additional mediational processes to adequate adjustment. The Semi-Structured Interview detailed below provided the data for these measures (see Appendices G and H). Child Adjustment Child adjustment was assessed with a parent-report measure, The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL-Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983), one of the most widely used measures for assessing children’s social competence and behavior problems. Of particular interest to this study were the areas of behavioral, social, and academic (scholastic) competence. First, parents or parent-figures report on the degree of their child’s involvement and success in social activities and relationships, and academic subjects. Second, parents complete a 113-item checklist by indicating the degree to which their child has demonstrated the described behavior during the previous 6 months, from 0 (indicating that the item is not true of the child) to 2 (indicating that the item is very true or often true of the child). The Child Behavior Profile is scored 59 from the CBCL and consists of three competence scales (activities, social, and school) and eight behavior problem scales derived from factor analysis (withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior). In addition, the behavior problem scales are divided into two broad-band factors labeled " internalizing " and "externalizing ". The internalizing category includes: withdrawn, somatic complaints, and anxious/depressed behaviors. The externalizing category consists of delinquent and aggressive behaviors. Normalized T-scores, based on populations of nonclinical samples, are available for the competence scales, behavior problem scales, and the broad-band factors aVI =50, SD =10). Discrirninative validity is demonstrated by significant differences on all social competence and behavior problem scores between clinical and nonclinical samples. Median test-retest reliability is .89 and interparent correlations for total behavior problems in a sample of 4-16 year-old children was .64 (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). Construct validity is also reportedly high (Achenbach, 1978; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1979). In this sample, mothers’ and fathers’ CBCL scores were strongly and significantly related. Pearson correlations between parents’ scores ranged from r= .37 to r=.84 (all with p’s< .01, based on 47 or 48 pairs), with a mean of r=.63. Interparent correlations for the internalizing and externalizing broad band factors were r(48)=.66, p< .001 and r(48)=.78, p< .001, respectively, and for total behavior problems was r(48)= .78, p < .001. Therefore, when both parents’ scores were available, they were averaged into a composite score for each scale. Semi-Structured Interview Both parents and children participated in an individual semi-structured interview (see Appendices G and H). The interview was developed for this study in order to explore the impact of stature on subjects’ development, personality, coping strategies, self-concept, and overall competence. All interviews were audio- or video- taped and tapes were coded by reading the interviewer’s abbreviated transcript notes while simultaneously watching and/or listening to the tapes. Interview Coding Scheme 1) Parent and Child Family History of SS: Parent and child interviews were coded for the presence or absence of a perceived family history of short stature. This was based on the individual’s subjective perceptions of height, and not on a clinical diagnosis or height estimate. The perceived presence of a family history of SS was also coded for proximity to the individual (e.g. , parent, sibling, grandparent). 2) Coping Strategies: Avoidance and approach coping strategies were coded for each parent and child interview. Avoidance coping included: distracting and avoiding, ignoring, emoting, etc. Approach coping included: problem-solving, seeking support, cognitive decision making, confrontion of an individual or situation, etc. See Appendices I and J, the "Coping Coding Manual" and sample "Coding Sheet," for a more detailed description of the coping categories and examples of strategies. Proportion scores based on each category of coping response divided by the total number of coping responses were calculated for each subject. Because there were only two categories of coping, the proportion scores were somewhat redundant. 61 Therefore, results will be discussed in terms of approach coping. 3) Mt of Stature on Adjustment: After reviewing a subset of interviews, it became apparent that stature could have both a positive and negative impact on subjects’ lives. In order to assess the unique contributions of each of these dimensions, two global assessments of the positive and negative impact of stature on parents’ and children’s adjustment and life experiences were coded. Positive and Negative Impact scores ranged from 0 (no positive/ negative impact) to 3 (strongly positive/ negative impact), and were assigned after coders reviewed the subject’s entire interview. Inter-rater Reliability The first step in coding the semi-structured interviews consisted of identifying coping strategies. The second step involved classifying each strategy as either Approach or Avoidance coping. Finally, coders assigned a Negative and Positive Impact of Stature score after reviewing each interview in its entirety. A clinical psychology graduate student served as the primary coder and an undergraduate senior psychology major, without knowledge of subjects’ group membership, served as a secondary coder. The secondary coder assisted in establishing and maintaining reliability, and once reliability was sustained, independently coded a subset of interviews. Training for coding coping strategies first involved reviewing, discussing, and understanding the coding manual, categories, and behavioral examples of the categories. Second, the coders reviewed, discussed and coded together a subset of 62 training interviews. Coders then independently coded a subset of training interviews which were followed by discussion and resolution of differences. As suggested by Gelfand and Hartman (1984), acceptable levels of reliability were considered at least .70 for coping strategies and coping categories, and an r of at least .60 for Negative and Positive Impact scores. Once these levels of reliability were maintained, coders independently coded interviews. The primary coder coded approximately five times as many interviews as the secondary coder. Rater drift was prevented by weekly accuracy checks in which the secondary coder coded about every fourth interview of the primary coder. Discrepancies in coding were settled through discussion. After coping strategies were identified, those which both coders agreed upon (after resolving differences) were rated as either Approach or Avoidance coping. Reliability for identifying coping strategies was calculated by the following formula suggested by Hakim-Larson and Hobart (1987) for segmenting speech units: number of perfect agreements/one-half the sum of Rater A’s total responses plus Rater B’s total responses. Inter—rater agreement on 24% of all the interviews was .70. Reliability for coping categories (perfect agreement/total units) was .96. The Pearson correlation coefficient between raters’ Negative Impact scores was r(39)=.74, p< .001, and for the Positive Impact scores was r(39)=.60, p< .001. Figure 2. Variables and Measures 63 VARIABLE Diagnostic/Demographic Parameters MEASURE Diagnosis Child and Family Information SES Questionnaire/Duncan TSEI2 Family Mediational Processes Family Support, Conflict, Control Parental Self-worth Parental Coping Styles Parental Impact of Own Stature Family Environment Scale Adult Self-Perception Profile Semi-Structured Interview Semi-Structured Interview Child Mediational Processes Self-worth Coping Styles Impact of Stature Self-Perception Profile for Children or Adolescents Semi-Structured Interview Semi-Structured Interview Child Adj ustrnent Social Competence Behavior Problems Academic Competence Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) CBCL CBCL CHAPTER 4 Results Statistical significance testing has been an area of considerable controversy, especially within the past decade. More recently, there has been a growing body of literature which criticizes the reliance of social science on significance testing, and suggests alternative methods for determining the significance or importance of research findings (see Carver, 1993; Judd, McClelland, & Culhane, 1995; Shaver, 1993; and Thompson, 1993). Kraemer and Thiemann (1988) emphasize that "specification of the critical effect size and the required power. . .must be realistic, not idealistic," (p.28). Similarly, Judd et al. (1995) suggest that "there is nothing sacred about an alpha level arbitrarily set at .05. When the sample size is small and constrained, an increase in power at somewhat greater risk of Type I error may be worthwhile. This is often true in applied research," (p. 445). These researchers also suggest that effect sizes, rather than levels of significance, be the focus of attention (Carver, 1993). Judd et al. (1995) add that even small effect sizes may be of "both theoretical and practical importance, " (p. 43 8). The data analyses of this study were guided by these arguments. Because the sample size was relatively small, it was deemed appropriate to increase power by setting the alpha level at .10. While this increased the chance of Type I error, the risks associated with it were judged to be less than those of Type II error. This decision was based partly in the fact that the body of research on SS and its impact on children and their families is quite limited. In addition, identifying areas of concern 65 has widespread implications for clinical intervention with children with SS and their families. flypptheses 1 apd 2: Group Differerm A series of one-way analyses of variance (AN OVA) was run in order to test the hypothesis that children and adolescents with SS would be rated by their parents as having more psychosocial and behavioral problems than children with normal stature (NS). The hypothesis was partially supported by the data (see Table 1). In general, children with SS have higher scores on all of the behavior problem scales than children with NS. These group differences are statistically significant for social problems, internalizing behavior problems, and total behavior problems, with the SS group exhibiting greater difficulties. In addition, as compared to children with NS, children with SS are rated as having significantly lower overall competence; this includes less involvement in extracurricular activities, having fewer close friends, and less frequent contact with the friends that they do have. No correction for multiple comparisons was made in these analyses, because the present study is concerned with results that are clinically significant. While this approach risked inflating Type I errors, the probability of this occuring was decreased by the small sample size and low power. Nevertheless, these results need to be interpreted with some caution. 66 Table 1. Group Comparisons for the Child Behavior Checklist‘ Short Stature Normal Stature n=33 SCALE Mean SD Mean SD Total Social Competence 45.92 6.90 53.88T‘l' 9.19 Activities 48.00 5.37 5039* 4.86 Social 43 .45 7.25 49.80H' 6. 14 School2 47.73 7.85 49.42 6.56 Social Problems 55.91 6.10 52.081L 3.68 Thought Problems 54.62 6.69 52.48 3.75 Attention Problems 55.91 8.68 53.56 5.83 Internalizing Behavior Probs. 51.35 10.00 45.94** 9.73 Social Withdrawal 53.52 6.22 51.95 3.39 Somatic Complaints 56.17 7.02 54.97 5.61 Anxious Depressed 54.82 7.45 52.70 4.28 Externalizing Behavior Probs. 49.77 10.29 46.33 10.01 Aggressive Behavior 54.41 7.23 53.05 6.05 Delinquent Behavior 53.98 6.17 52.55 4.63 Total Behavior Problems 51.35 10.54 44.77** 11.19 ‘Composite T-scores averaged for mothers and fathers when both available. 2n=32 for the short stature group on this scale. *p< .10 *"‘p< .05 ’rp< .01 TTp< .001 67 Another series of one-way analyses of variance was run to test the hypothesis that children with 88 would have lower self-competence than children with NS (see Table 2). On all but one of the Self-Perception Profile scales, children with SS rated themselves as having lower competence, with significant differences between groups for global self-worth, athletic competence, and physical appearance. Thus, the hypothesis was partially supported by the data. Of note is the fact that children with SS rated themselves as having somewhat more socially appropriate behavior than children with NS, although this difference was not statistically significant. Similar to the analyses above, these results need to be interpreted carefully as no correction was made for multiple comparisons. Table 2. Group Comparisgs for me Self-Perception Profile Short Stature Normal Stature n=32 n=33 SCALE Mean SD Mean SD Global Self-Worth 3.15 .61 3.46b .57 Athletic Competence 2.74 .86 3.15b .57 Behavioral Conduct 3.14 .58 2.98 .70 Physical Appearance 2.76 .80 3.06a .56 Scholastic Competence 3.08 .74 3.16 .66 Social Acceptance 2.93 .64 3.19 .68 ‘p< .10 bp< .05 68 Further (post-hoe) analyses were performed to assess differences between the two groups in coping, the impact of height, and selfldescriptions of height. A two- tailed unpaired t-test was conducted to test for group differences in coping strategies. There was no difference between children with SS and NS in their proportional use of coping strategies (t=.63, df= 39, p=.53). Thus, children with SS were no more likely than children of NS to use approach or avoidant coping strategies. With regard to stature, there was no significant difference between the two groups in their positive impact scores (t=.19, df =58, p=.85). However, compared to children of NS, children with SS received significantly higher negative impact scores (t=7.02, df= 50.85, p < .001) and more frequently described themselves as below average in height (t=6.47, df=44.88, p< .001). Hypothesis 3: Gender Differences The hypothesis that boys with SS would demonstrate greater psychosocial and behavior problems than girls with SS and children with N8 was assessed in two ways. First, a 2 X 2 analysis of variance (AN OVA) was run to examine gender by stature interaction effects and gender main effects. This initial step was performed because of a potential confound in comparing boys with SS and girls with SS to children with NS. Any observed differences in this comparison could be due to general gender differences, rather than to differences specific to children with SS. This preliminary analysis revealed no significant gender by stature interaction effects. Hence, a series of one-way analyses of variance followed, comparing the three groups (SS boys, SS 69 girls, and NS children) on the outcome variables (all of the CBCL scales and factors, and the Self-Perception Profile scales). As demonstrated by Tables 3, 4, and 5, the hypothesis is only partly supported by the data. Tables 3, 4 and 5 display only the analyses resulting in significant F-values (p < .10). For the CBCL behavior scales (see Table 3), significant between-group differences (p < .05) were found for Total Behavior Problems and Social Problems. Boys with SS had significantly more behavior problems than children with NS, however girls with SS did not differ significantly from either group. Girls with SS had the most internalizing behavior problems, but between-group differences did not reach the .05 criterion on this factor. Boys with SS also demonstrated more significant social problems than children with NS, while girls with SS demonstrated levels of social problems between, but not significantly different from, the other two groups. On the competence scales of the CBCL, significant F-values were found for activities and social competence (see Table 4). Girls with SS participated in significantly fewer extracurricular activities than boys with SS and children with NS, who did not differ significantly from each other on this scale. Boys and girls with SS had similar levels of social competence, which were significantly lower than that of children with NS. Table 3. 70 .O_ne-Wav Anplvsis of Variance (ANOVA) for Stature X Gender Groups and CBCL Behavior Problem Scales CBCL Scales Total Behavior Internalizing Social Problems* Problems* Behavior Problems _pGrou N M. i). M. _S_D M £2 SS Boys 23 51.33 9.54 51.30 8.49 56.33 6.35 SS Girls 10 51.40 13.14 51.45 13.38 54.95 5.66 NS 33 44.77 11.19 45.94 9.73 52.08 3.68 Children F 2.97a 2.443 5.00b *SS Boys are significantly different from NS Children at the .05 level, while SS Girls do not differ significantly from the other two groups. ap<.10 bp<.01 71 Table 4. One-Way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) for Stature X Gender Groups and CBCL Competence Scales CBCL Scales Activities* Social” _nGrou N M. SD M _82 SS Boys 23 49.20 4.41 43.13 7.08 SS Girls 10 45.25 6.55 44.20 7 .98 NS 33 50.39 4.86 49.80 6.14 Children F 4.07b 7.36c *SS Girls are significantly different from SS Boys and NS Children at the .05 level, while SS Boys and NS Children do not differ significantly from each other. **NS Children are significantly different from SS Boys and SS Girls at the .05 level, while SS Boys and SS Girls do not differ significantly from each other. ap<.10 bp<.05 cp<.01 72 Table 5. One-Way Analysis of Variance (AN OVA) for Stature X Gender Groups and Self- Perception Scales Self-Perception Scales Athletic Comp_etence Physical Appearance* _nGrou M M _S_D M ill SS Boys 23 2.78 .77 2.88 .87 SS Girls 10 2.65 1.06 2.51 .59 NS 33 3.15 .57 3.06 .56 Children F 2.65“ 2.49“ *SS Girls are significantly different from NS Children at the .05 level, while SS boys do not differ significantly from the other two groups. ap<.10 73 On the Self-Perception Profile, significant F-values were found for the athletic competence and physical appearance scales (see Table 5). While group differences on the athletic competence scale did not reach significance at the p < .05 level, girls with SS rated themselves the lowest, children with NS the highest, and boys with SS rated themselves between the two. On the physical appearance scale, girls with SS rated themselves significantly lower than children with NS, while boys with SS rated themselves between the two groups and did not differ significantly from either of them. Hypothesis 4: Age Differences The hypothesis that younger children (under 12 years) with SS would demonstrate greater self-competence, but also greater psychosocial and behavior problems than older children and adolescents with SS (12 years and older), was only partially supported by the data. A series of one-way analyses of variance assessed group differences on the child outcome measures. On the Self-Perception Profile for Children and Adolescents, the younger group rated themselves significantly higher on athletic competence (F[1,30]15.55, p< .001), physical appearance (F[,30]4.73, p< .05), behavioral conduct (F[1,30]=3.37, p< .10), and global self-worth (F[1,30] =3.25, p< .10). There were no significant differences between the groups on the scholastic competence or social acceptance scales of this profile. In addition, contrary to the hypothesis, there were no significant age group differences on any of the competence and behavior problem scales and factors of the CBCL. A post-hoe 74 two—tailed unpaired t-test revealed no significant difference between the age groups in their use of approach or avoidant coping strategies. Hypothesis 5: Family Histog of SS It was predicted that children who have either a perceived or diagnosed family history of SS would demonstrate better psychosocial adjustment than those without a perceived or diagnosed family history of SS. This hypothesis was tested in two sets of analyses, examining perceptions of familial short stature (FSS) separate from diagnosed FSS. The semi-structured interview provided data regarding children’s perceptions of family members’ stature. Of the 30 children with SS who participated in the interview, only 6 of them did not perceive a family member (e.g., parent, sibling, grandparent, etc.) as below average in height. A series of one-way analyses of variance provided comparisons between children with SS with and without a perceived family history of SS on the outcome measures (CBCL and Self-Perception Profile scales). N 0 significant group differences were found except for one, which was in the opposite direction to that which was predicted. On the Self-Perception Profile, children with SS who reported a perceived family history of SS rated themselves as less satisfied with their physical appearance than those without a perceived family history of SS (F[l,27]=5.20, p< .05). In order to test the second half of Hypothesis 4, a series of one-way analyses of variance was run to assess differences on the outcome measures between the 2 diagnostic groups of children with SS who did not have a family history of SS (CD 75 and GHD) and the 2 diagnostic groups that m (FSS and CD/FSS). Only one between-group difference was found. Children with a family history of SS were rated significantly higher on the CBCL scale of social competence than those without FSS (F[1,30] =7.12, p< .05). Comparable results were found when the one-way AN OVA’s were run comparing all four diagnostic groups on the outcome measures. Children with FSS had the highest social competence scores, whereas children with CD had the lowest (F[3,28] =2.94, p=.05). The difference between these two groups was significant at the p < .05 level. Children with GHD had the second lowest scores and children with CD/FSS had the second highest scores on the social competence scale, but no other groups were significantly different from one another. Therefore, examining the 4 diagnostic groups separately did not mask the effect, and it provided more specific data regarding group differences. Hypothesis 6: Devaluing Physical Appearance One-way analyses of variance were also used to test the hypothesis that children with SS who devalued physical appearance would have greater self-esteem and fewer internalizing behavior problems than children with SS who felt that physical appearance was more important. The Harter Self-Perception Profile Importance Rating Scale provided the measure of children’s value of physical appearance. The children with SS were divided into a devaluing group (n=21) and a valuing group (n=11) using a median split on the importance of physical appearance rating (devalue <2.75