THESIS J l 1' MI ICHIG GAN STATEU ! ll’lllzll llllllJill”llllllllllllll 93 01555 9333 ll This is to certify that the dissertation entitled MISSED MESSAGES: HUN PKDSPECI‘IVE TEACHEIB' RACIAL ATI'ITUIES MEDIATE WHAT THEY LEAm FROM A COUIBE (N DIVERSITY presented by M. ARIHURCAHIJN has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. " degree in Educational Psychology MMW Major professor Date W 0;. ”‘96 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LIBRARY Mlchlgan State Unlverslty PLACE IN RETURN BOX to move this checkout from your roeord. TO AVOID FINES Mum on or baton date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE G7 ufll MARJfimi MSUIsAn.‘.“‘ . " ’1 ‘ w ...-.....M. MISSED MESSAGES: HOW PROSPECTIVE TEACHERS’ RACIAL ATTITUDES MEDIATE WHAT THEY LEARN FROM A COURSE ON DIVERSITY By M. Arthur Gannon A DISSERTA'I'ION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF COUNSELING, EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, AND SPECIAL EDUCATION 1996 ABSTRACT MISSED MESSAGES: HOW PROSPECTIVE TEACHERS’ RACIAL ATTITUDES MEDIATE WHAT THEY IEARN FROM A COURSE ON DIVERSITY BY M. ARTHUR GARMON This study investigated the impact of a multicultural education course on the racial attitudes and beliefs of a group of approximately 200 prospective teachers. One primary objective was to identify students' entering attitudes and beliefs relative to racial/ethnic minority groups and then to determine whether these attitudes and beliefs changed during the course of the semester. A second objective was to determine whether students who expreswd initially favorable racial attitudes differed substantially from those who expressed initially unfavorable attitudes. Data sources for the study were pre and post administrations of a racial attitude survey and a teaching/learning questionnaire. In addition, a subset of the participants were interviewed at two-week intervals throughout the semester. The major finding of the study was that students’ entering racial attitudes and beliefs appeared to mediate what they learned from the course. More specifically, on several diversity issues addressed in the course the beliefs of high students (those who began the course with more favorable racial attitudes) were significantly more likely to change in the desired direction than were the beliefs of low students (those who began the course with less favorable racial attitudes); most of the low students seemed to miss the key messages that the course intended to convey. Also, the researcher found that 1) high students were significantly more likely to report more and higher quality interracial friendships than low students, 2) high students showed greater awareness of racial prejudice and discrimination than did low students, and 3) there were no differences between the two groups of students in terms of their attendance, their performance on course exams, their final course grades, or their overall satisfaction with the course. Copyfight by M. ARTHUR GARMON 1996 This dissertation is dedicated to my parents, Rev. Moses and Ada M. Gannon, in loving appreciation of their unfailing love, support, and encouragement ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS In the writing of a dissertation, the old adage “No man [person] is an island” certainly holds true. I know that it would have been extremely difficult, if not impossible, for me to have finished this dissertation without the help and support of a lot of wonderful people. First, I would like to thank Jim Gavelek, my advisor, for guiding me through my doctoral program and into my dissertation. I’m sure that I was not an easy person to be an advisor for, and I appreciate his time, his patience, and his good advice. For a long time I was unsure about what I wanted to do when I finished my program, and my conversations with Jim really helped me think about my options and the pros and cons of each. Thanks to Carol Sue Englert for being a great mentor and friend to me throughout my years at Michigan State University. I deeply appreciate her advice, encouragement, and general support of my efforts; the interest and caring that she demonstrated in me and my endeavors even though she was not on either of my committees meant a lot to me. In addition, the research skills that I learned from working with her on the Early Literacy Project were extremely valuable to me during my dissertation process. I am also appreciative of the opportunities that she provided me (and all of her research assistants) to get publications and presentations. Thanks to Stan Trent for serving on my dissertation committee and for being another mentor and friend for the past few years. I appreciate his continuous encouragement and support of my efforts to complete my dissertation. I appreciate, too, the opportunities that he tried to make available to me for getting my work published in journals or presented at conferences. I also learned a lot from working and talking with him about CEP 240 and about the data I collected. Thanks to Lynn Paine and Lauren Young for serving on my program committee and on my dissertation committee. I appreciate not only their input into the planning of my program and the completing of my dissertation, but also their general support and willingness to be of help. I owe Lauren an extra debt of gratitude for inviting me to become involved with Team 3. This involvement provided me with valuable experiences which helped me recognize some of my talents and begin to clarify my professional goals. Also, I greatly appreciate her belief in me and in my abilities. A special thanks to my dissertation advisor, Linda Anderson, who was really great to work with. She held high expectations for me but also continually demonstrated her belief that I could rise to meet those expectations. She was very generous with her time, meeting with me regularly for a year and a half to discuss my progress (or lack thereol). Our many conversations were invaluable in helping me design, implement, and then write up my research. Linda offered many helpful suggestions all along the way, and she also was critical when she needed to be, but always in a very gentle way. Furthermore, it seemed that whenever I was feeling discouraged and/or overwhelmed, she always had something supportive, encouraging, and reassuring to say. Linda was everything (and more!) that anyone could ever hope to have for a dissertation chair, and I can’t thank her enough! Thanks to Gene Pemell for allowing me to serve as a teaching assistant for CEP 240 and for giving me the freedom to pursue my research agenda. I would also like to thank him for the support he has given me and for the great confidence he has always shown in my abilities. Thanks to Susan Melnick for helping me decide on the focus for my dissertation and to Len Bianchi for helping me with my statistical analyses. iii Thanks to all of my fellow graduate students who worked, played, and commiserated with me throughout my program. I don’t know what I would have done without their friendship and support. Specifically, thanks to Troy Mariage for our many debates/discussions and for modeling excellence in research and teaching. Thanks to Kathi Tarrant for being such a good friend from the beginning and for always believing in me. Thanks to Nancy Knapp for all of our stimulating discussions. Thanks to Jeff Sonnega for making me relax by taking me to parties and sporting events, especially basketball games. Thanks to Fenice Boyd, Marcia Fetters, and Teresa Scurto for their friendship and for the various ways that they supported me during my program. Thanks to Tanja Bisesi, Fenice Boyd, Cindy Brock, Ginny Goatley, Mary Rozendal, and Joyce Urba for participating in dissertation study groups with me; these were helpful. Thanks to Cindy Brock, Ginny Goatley, Mary Rozendal, and Steve Sheldon for the fun and laughter of our Saturday night card games that gave all of us a break from the pressures of our doctoral program. Finally, I want to offer an additional word of thanks to Mary Rozendal, who absolutely has a heart of gold. I appreciate immensely not only all the different ways in which she has helped me but also all that she continually does for others. For their continuing encouragement, support, and understanding, thanks to all the members of my family and to many of my old friends. In particular I want to thank three of my former students--Cari Filbrandt, Nikkole McNally, and Kathryn Overton--who also attended MSU and whose friendship did much to sustain me during my years here. Thanks also to my former colleagues at Indiana University South Bend for encouraging and supporting my efforts to complete my dissertation, especially Marie Doyle, who has been a truly wonderful and supportive friend. Finally, thanks to all the other professors, staff, friends, and fellow students who contributed in various ways to my completing this dissertation and who helped in making my years at Michigan State so enjoyable. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................ vii LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................. viii CHAPTER ONE - STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM .................................... 1 CHAPTER TWO - REVIEW OF RELATED RESEARCH ................................ 7 Definitions ................................................................................. 7 The Learning of Preservice Teachers .................................................. 10 Impact of Teacher Education on Students' Beliefs and Attitudes .......... 11 Influence of Students' Attitudes and Beliefs on Their Learning ........... l4 Racial Attitudes and Beliefs ............................................................. 20 Influence on Perception of New Information . 3. ............................. 20 The Persistence of Negative Racial Attitudes ................................ 22 Influence on Behavior toward Racial Minorities ............................ 26 Multicultural Teacher Education ........................................................ 28 Why Are the Changing Demographic Trends Problematic? ................ 29 Cultural Mismatches ................................................... 30 Different Learning Styles .............................................. 33 Negative Racial Attitudes and Beliefs ................................ 36 Conclusions ............................................................. 38 What Does It Mean to Prepare Teachers for Diversity? ..................... 39 Biography (Self -Awareness) ......................................... 39 Cultural Knowledge ................................................... 4O Instructional Strategies ................................................ 42 Field Experiences ...................................................... 43 Attitude Change ........................................................ 44 Conclusions ...................................................................... 47 CHAPTER THREE - METHODS ............................................................. 48 Purpose .................................................................................... 48 Research Design and Rationale ......................................................... 50 Course ..................................................................................... 51 Participants ................................................................................ 55 Data Sources .............................................................................. 56 Teaching/Learning Questionnaire .............................................. 56 Attitude Survey .................................................................. 57 Structured Interview ............................................................ 58 Unstructured Interviews ........................................................ 59 Data Analysis ............................................................................. 60 Quantitative Data ................................................................ 60 Attitude Surveys ........................................................ 60 Teaching/Learning Questionnaires ................................... 61 Course Records ........................................................ 62 Qualitative Data ........................................................................... 63 Structured Interviews ........................................................... 63 Unstructured Interviews ........................................................ 66 CHAPTER FOUR - RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ...................................... 68 Quantitative Data ......................................................................... 68 The Quick Discrimination Index ............................................... 69 All Students ............................................................. 69 Volunteer Students ..................................................... 7S ABCD Questionnaire ........................................................... 79 Course Records ................................................................. 87 Qualitative Data ........................................................................... 88 Target Students .................................................................. 89 Background Experiences .............................................. 89 Beliefs about the Education of Minorities ........................... 95 Impact of Course ....................................................... 104 A Close-up Look at Three Students ........................................... 121 Student Profiles ........................................................ 122 How Students Reacted to Course Content .......................... 127 The Influence of Personal Qualities .................................. 141 CHAPTER FIVE - SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS ........... 160 Summary of Major Findings ............................................................ 160 Quantitative Data ................................................................ 160 Qualitative Data .................................................................. 162 Discussion ................................................................................ 164 Beliefs and Attitudes as Mediating Factors ................................... 164 Processing of New Information ...................................... 165 Consistency Theories .................................................. 168 Belief /Attitude Strength ................................................ 170 Missed Messages ................................................................ 171 Other Learnings ................................................................. 173 Conclusions ............................................................................... 178 Limitations of the Study ................................................................. 183 Implications ............................................................................... 184 Future Research .......................................................................... 189 APPENDICES Appendix A - CEP 240 Syllabus, Reading List, and Sample Activities ........... 191 Appendix B - ABCD Questionnaire .................................................... 200 Appendix C - Attitude Surveys ......................................................... 211 Appendix D - Structured Interviews ................................................... 214 REFERENCES .................................................................................... 217 LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1 - Participants in the Study ............................................................ 55 Table 3.2 - Data Sources for Participants in Study ............................................ 56 Table 4.7 - Student Responses to QDI Items .................................................. 72 Table 4.2 - Volunteer Students’ Pre and Post Scores on the QDI ........................... 76 Table 4.3 - Comparison of Volunteers and Nonvolunteers on Selected Variables ........ 77 Table 4.4a - Student Responses to ABCD items #1-14 ....................................... 80 Table 4.4b - Student Responses to ABCD items #15-22 ................................... 81-82 Table 4.4c - Student Responses to ABCD items #23-25 ..................................... 83 Table 4.5 - Change Scores for High and Lows on Selected ABCD Items ................. 87 Table 4.6 - Mean Scores for Attendance, Midterm Exam, Final Exam, and Final Grade ................................................................ 88 Table 4.7 - Target Students ...................................................................... 89 Table 4.8 - Explanations of Lower Minority School Achievement .......................... 100 Table 4.9 - Most Important Learnings .......................................................... 107 Table 4.10 - Most/Least Valuable Class Sessions ............................................. 110 Table 4.11 - Students’ Feelings about the Course ............................................. 111 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 4.1 - Distribution of scores on Quick Discrimination Index (QDI) ................ 7O viii CHAPTER ONE STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Repeatedly in the current teacher education literature, one reads that during the next 25 years the rapidly changing demographic composition of our country is going to result in an increasingly diverse student population (Pallas, Natriello, and McDill, 1989), and yet the projections are that the teaching force will wntinue to be primarily White, monolingual, and female (Grant & Secada, 1990; Zeichner, 1993). In response to these two trends, many educators (e.g., see Dilworth, 1993; Grant, 1992) are arguing that teacher education programs must do more to better prepare teacher candidates for teaching a diverse student population. In its 1972 publication, No One Model American, the American Association of Colleges of Teacher Education set forth four multicultural goals for teacher education programs and asserted that ”The commitment to cultural pluralism must permeate all areas of the educational experience provided for prospective teachers " (cited in Gollnick, 1992, p. 225). In 1979 the National Council for the Accreditation of Teacher Education (NCATE) added multicultural criteria to its standards for all institutions seeking national accreditation. In Tomorrow's Schools the Holmes Group (1990) also stressed the importance of teachers learning to teach ”everybody's children.” Clearly, teacher educators are aware of and attempting to respond to the challenge. Nevertheless, attempts to increase the multicultural emphasis in teacher education programs, however well-intentioned, are fraught with difficulties. First, a fundamental requisite for effective problem-solving is to clearly define the problem; however, it is not at all apparent that teacher educators have a clear sense of the problem they are attempting to address. The most frequently cited argument in support of the need to prepare teachers for diversity is the growing disparity between a mostly White and female teaching force and an increasingly diverse student population. It has not been made clear, however, whether the problem is the cultural mismatch between teachers and students, the prospective teachers' lack of knowledge and skills in teaching groups different from themselves, their attitudes toward and beliefs about other racial/ethnic groups, or some combination of these and other factors. A second difficulty is that teacher educators seem to have given insufficient attention to defining and then justifying what it is they seek to develop in prospective teachers-awareness, sensitivity, knowledge, skills, beliefs, attitudes, etc. These terms abound in the literature as characteristics that preservice teachers need to develop; however, seldom are they defined adequately, nor is it always explained why the development of these characteristics is an appropriate response to the problem. Third, as a consequence of the different conceptions of the problem and the different conceptions of what prospective teachers need to develop, there seems to be little consensus on how to go about providing multicultural teacher education (In refening to the process of preparing prospective teachers to work with diverse learners, I will use multicultural teacher education as synonymous with preparing teachers for diversity.) In his review of the literature in this area, Zeichner (1993) identified five different approaches to multicultural teacher education, approaches that, while they might well be considered complementary, clearly reflect the differing perspectives on the problem and on what prospective teachers need to develop. Thus, while there is strong agreement that multicultural teacher education is needed and that teacher education programs must provide it, there appears to be less agreement on how to go about doing so. Finally, some educators question whether teaching for diversity is something that can be taught to prospective teachers and whether most efforts to do so are truly effective or appropriate responses to the continuing problems of poor and minority school children. Haberman (1991), for instance, argues that teacher education programs are not powerful enough to change students' deep-seated beliefs and values, while Gay (1983) contends that including issues of class, gender, and religion diverts attention away from ethnicity, which she regards as the major issue. Others (e. g., Gibson, 1984; Hilliard, 1974) have argued that multicultural education is paternalistic and that culturally different children require no special pedagogy. Each of these fundamental issues will have to be addressed if teacher education programs are to do a more effective job of preparing teachers to work with diverse learners. Needed is more research to shed additional light on the aforementioned difficulties. In their review of the literature on preparing teachers for diversity, Grant and Secada (1990) observed: The downside of the research reviewed here, like so much research in education, is that it does not offer enough detail or conclusive enough evidence to argue that these are effective ways of offering multicultural preservice education. . . . we still need more information about the scope of effective educational practice and the combinations of practice that will result in optimal outcomes” (p. 413) To learn more about the effectiveness of multicultural teacher education, I decided to examine a single course and attempt to determine its apparent impact on students. In looking at one course which seemed to have goals consistent with many multicultural teacher education courses, I focused on whether and how students' racial attitudes and beliefs mediated their experience of the course and their learning from the course. While the present study does not directly address any of the difficulties identified in the previous paragraph, it still contributes to the field in some worthwhile ways. First, it provides a close look at the structure and content of one multicultural education course, at how a selected group of students experience the course, and at what they learn from it. Second, this study examines the role of racial attitudes and beliefs in the learning of prospective teachers. Third, it offers data to speak to the question of whether or not a multicultural teacher education course affects the racial attitudes and beliefs that prospective teachers bring to the course. Finally, some of the data collected for this study may suggest ways to increase the effectiveness of multicultural teacher education courses. In addition to the literature on preparing teachers for diversity, this study was also informed by research on leaming to teach and by research on racial attitudes. Several reviewers of the learning to teach literature (Borko & Putnam, in press; Kagan, 1992; and Richardson, 1996) have concluded that what prospective teachers learn from their teacher education courses is strongly influenced by the beliefs and attitudes which they bring to those courses. Recognizing their immense importance, Pajares (1992) has argued that ”investigating the educational beliefs of teachers and teacher candidates should become a focus of current educational research” (p. 322). This study contributes to the literature by examining prospective teachers' beliefs about the education of racial minority students. Meanwhile, researchers on racial attitudes (Duckitt, 1992; Maluso, 1995; Oskamp, 1991, Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993) have reported that many Whites still seem to possess negative attitudes toward racial minorities and that these attitudes influence not only how they interpret information about racial minorities, but also how they might behave toward them. Given the apparent importance of entering beliefs and attitudes, coupled with the fact that most prospective teachers are White while an increasing proportion of our nation's public school students are racial minorities, I believe that any efforts to prepare teachers for diversity must take into account their racial beliefs and attitudes, because it seems likely that these beliefs and attitudes will influence what they learn. Therefore, I decided to make prospective teachers' racial attitudes the major focus in my dissertation research. Three major questions guided this study: 1. How are students who express favorable attitudes toward minority groups similar to and different from students who express unfavorable attitudes? 2. To what extent do students' attitudes toward minority groups change during the course? 3. Are there important differences between students with initially favorable racial attitudes and those with initially unfavorable ones in terms of their perceptions of the course or their learning from the course? First, I wanted to determine whether preservice teachers who express initially favorable attitudes toward minority groups differ substantially from those who express initially unfavorable attitudes and also whether there appear to be important differences in what they learn from the course. In her study of preservice teachers in a multicultural education course, McGeehan (1982) found that students who began the course with positive feelings toward other ethnic groups were the ones who gained the most multicultural knowledge during the course. Similarly, Ross and Smith (1992) reported that preservice teachers' learning outcomes seemed to be related to the strength of their entering perspectives. Additionally, they found that students who reported more pre- college experiences with minorities seemed to display more openness to the course content. The findings of these researchers are consistent with the finding frequently reported in the learning-to—teach literature, that prospective teachers' entering beliefs influence what they learn from their teacher education classes. The findings further suggest that the prior interracial experiences may also be an important variable. Therefore, in my dissertation study I attempted to determine whether, with this sample of students, particular background characteristics distinguished those with favorable attitudes from those with unfavorable attitudes. Second, I sought to identify prospective teachers' entering attitudes toward racial/ethnic minority groups, and then to determine how and to what extent their attitudes and their knowledge/beliefs did or did not change during the course of the semester. Noordhoff and Kleinfeld (1993) reported that a missing piece in their research was an examination of their preservice teachers' entering views about diverse students and their beliefs about teaching them. They argued that such an examination is important because teacher learning is not an additive process, but rather a process of conceptual change. Only by knowing where a person started will we be able to determine how he/she has changed. Third, I wanted to document several participants' perceptions of their experience in the course, as well as their perceptions of what they learned from the course. Grant and Secada (1990) asserted that "A basic tenet of education is that instruction should follow development. Yet we have no maps of how teacher cognitions, beliefs, and skills with respect to the teaching of diverse student populations actually develop” (p. 419). Hopefully, this study will assist us in beginning to develop such maps. I will conclude this chapter by presenting a brief overview of the remaining chapters in this report. In Chapter 2, I review three relevant bodies of literature: the learning-to- teach literature, focusing on the important role that entering attitudes and beliefs have in preservice teachers' learning; the literature on racial attitudes, focusing on the continued existence of unfavorable attitudes by many White Americans toward racial minorities; and the literature on multicultural teacher education, focusing on the various strategies currently being employed to prepare preservice teachers for diversity. In Chapter 3, I describe the methods used in the study. I begin by explaining the design of the study and my rationale for it. Then I describe the course that is being studied, the participants in the study, and the various instruments that were used. Finally, I explain how I analyzed the data that were collected, first the quantitative data and then the qualitative data. In Chapter 4, I report the results of this investigation. I first present the results of the quantitative data collected from all the students in the class and then that collected from a smaller group of volunteers. Next, I discuss the results of my analysis of the qualitative data for a group of target students selected from among the volunteers. Finally, I discuss additional qualitative data on three focus students selected from among the target students. In Chapter 5, I summarize the major findings of this research. After some discussion, I then draw a number of conclusions based on my major findings and suggest implications that they would seem to have for teacher educators. Finally, I discuss limitations of the study and my recommendations for future research. CHAPTER TWO REVIEW OF RELATED RESEARCH Prior research in three broad areas seems to bear on the present research. I will review the research on the leaming of preservice teachers, racial attitudes and beliefs, and multicultural teacher education . I will begin by defining some of the key terms that I will be using. Then, in the second section of the review, I will discuss how, during the past two decades, research on learning to teach has indicated that the perspectives with which prospective teachers begin their preparation strongly influence what and how much they leam during their programs. In the third section I will discuss how the racial attitudes and beliefs that people hold have been shown to influence not only their receptiveness to new information but also their behavior toward certain groups of people. In addition, I will present research which indicates that racial prejudice and discrimination are still problems in American society. In the final section I will discuss different explanations of the need for multicultural teacher education and different approaches that are being advocated. I will argue that the biggest emphasis in current approaches to multicultural teacher education seems to be on providing prospective teachers with information about diversity, and I will make the case that we need to pay more attention to their racial attitudes and beliefs and to how they might mediate what is (or is not) learned from courses on diversity. Definitions Because this study deals with attitudes and beliefs and how they may influence the learning of prospective teachers, I need to lay some groundwork for the literature review by first defining these key terms. I have found that terms like attitudes and beliefs abound in the literature on learning to teach, but seldom is either of these terms clearly defined. 1 am sure that part of the reason for this is that these are difficult terms to define precisely. For example, Fishbein and Azjen (cited in McGuire, 1986) reviewed 200 research studies in social psychology and found 500 different operational definitions of attitude. McGuire has detailed the many difficulties involved in attempting to define this term with precision, but for the purposes of this present discussion, social psychological precision seems less important than simplicity and clarity. Therefore, I will define attitude simply as an ”evaluative reaction toward something or someone" (Myers, 1990, p. 34). Some social psychologists (e.g., Myers, 1990; Sampson, 1991; Triandis, 1971) have conceived of an attitude as being composed of three interrelated components: a cognitive component, consisting of the person's beliefs about the attitude object; an affective component, consisting of the person's feelings toward the attitude object; and a behavioral component, consisting of the person's behavior toward the attitude object. Other social psychologists (e.g., Duckitt, 1992; Lott, 1995; Oskamp, 1991; Petty & Cacioppo, 1981) choose to regard these three components as separate entities, and they use the term attitude to refer only to the person's feelings toward someone or something. According to Duckitt (1992), the tri-componential view of attitude was once widely- accepted; however, during the past two decades this view seems to have fallen out of favor among most social psychologists. One reason for the alleged shift in perspective was that “the interrelationship among the three components and how they influenced behavior were never definitively clarified” (p. 12). Lott (1995) identified several other problems with the tri-componential view, including the ”difficulty of assessing an attitude independently of its presumed components” (p. 21). Similarly, Maluso (1995) argued that the research evidence suggests that the three components might most appropriately be regarded as independent (though interrelated) constructs. Therefore, in this paper I will adopt social psychologists' prevailing view of attitude as being an affective construct distinct from beliefs and behaviors. Belief, like attitude, is a difficult construct to define precisely. In his review of the literature on teachers' beliefs, Pajares (1992) discussed the difficulty of defining this term and the additional difficulties in distinguishing beliefs from knowledge; however, in his article he never provided a clear definition of belief, other than at one point saying that it is ”an individual's judgment of the truth or falsity of a proposition” (p. 316). I find that I am uncomfortable with his use of the word judgment in this definition because it could imply that beliefs are necessarily evaluative, but I do not believe this to be the case. Bern (1970), for example, distinguishes between beliefs and evaluative beliefs. He explains that while many beliefs are evaluative in nature (e. g., Spinach has a terrible taste), not all of them are (e.g., Cancer can cause death). Additionally, Pajares' (1992) use of the term judgment also seems to imply that the acquisition of beliefs is a far more rational process than it actually is. Bem (1970) has suggested that many belief -- particularly those developed as a result of direct experience or through the influence of some hi ghly-valued external authority--are often accepted without question. In other words, the individual does not engage in making judgments about the truth or falsity of the information presented; he/she simply accepts it as truth. Bern used the term primitive beliefs to refer to "beliefs which demand no independent formal or empirical confirmation and which require no justification beyond a brief citation of direct experience” (p. 6). These seem to be two serious shortcomings of Pajares' (1992) definition of belief. Petty and Cacioppo (1981) defined belief as “the information that a person has about other people, objects, and issues" (p. 7). Bar-Tal (1990), after discussing a number of different definitions proposed by various social psychologists, chose to define a belief as "a proposition to which a person attributes at least a minimal degree of confidence'I (p. 14). The definition which I prefer because it is simple but clear was offered by Scheibe (1970); he defined a belief as what a person ”considers to be true or likely” (p. l). 10 Finally, the terms racial attitudes and racial beliefs are defined in the same way as attitudes and beliefs, except that they relate specifically to issues of race. I have explained how I am defining the key terms appearing in this paper, but the reader needs to understand that other researchers whose work I will be citing in the following pages may be defining these terms differently, or they may use other terms in place of these. The Learning of Preservice Teachers One of the assumptions underlying efforts to prepare teachers for diversity is that ”teaching for diversity” is something that can be taught. In the literature the implicit belief of those researching and writing about this issue is exceedingly clear, because the question of whether or not "teaching for diversity” can be taught is almost never raised. This is unfortunate, and rather surprising, considering Zeichner's (1993) observation: Very little evidence exists in the literature that the changes documented by teacher educators are long lasting or that they influence the way in which prospective teachers actually teach. Generally, we know very little about the development of teacher education students' cognitions, beliefs, and skills with respect to the teaching of diverse learners, including how particular teacher education strategies influence teacher learning” (pp. 20-21) Some of the research conducted during the past two decades suggests that there are limits to what can be accomplished through teacher education; a clear awareness and understanding of what and where those limits might be could enable us to make our instructional efforts even more effective by allowing us to concentrate most of our efforts in the areas where they can have the most powerful, far-reaching, and enduring effects. Haberman (1991) has argued that cultural awareness is not something that can be taught by teacher education programs because it involves changing a person's values. He contended that the evidence on whether colleges and universities can change students' values is I'spotty and inconclusive.” Haberman defined beliefs as ”what we think, "attitudes as "what we feel,‘ and values as “the beliefs and attitudes we hold strongly enough to act upon" (p. 25). He elaborated by saying that ”values represent our very essence as people reflecting what we are, what we expect, and how we explain the world around us" (p. 25). The research on the impact that colleges and universities have on 11 their students' values has been ignored by teacher educators, Haberman claimed. People's values are acquired over time from significant others during childhood and adolescence. These values, once acquired, ”serve as lifelong guides to behavior" and are not easily changed. Haberman's contention is supported by Grant and Secada's (1990) report on a study by Grant and Grant which assessed the attitudes of participants in a multicultural institute. These researchers found that ”people's attitudes and behaviors about multicultural education are not easily changed and that extended training is necessary” (p. 417). Haberman (1991) also asserted, Apparently, teacher educators must be assuming 1) that their preservice students' values are unimportant or irrelevant to the type of teachers they will become; or 2) that their students all enter the university with the values they will need to become effective teachers in a multicultural society, or 3) that the teacher education program will be sufficiently powerful to change all their students in ways that will enable them to act on the ”right” values" (p. 27). There is considerable research that seems to support Haberman's position, suggesting not only that teacher education courses have little impact on students' attitudes and beliefs, but also that their entering attitudes and beliefs actually influence what and how much they learn in their courses. Immct of Teacher Education on Students' Beliefs and Attitudes Some research reports that teacher education courses appear to have little influence on prospective teachers. In his classic “Five Towns" study, Lortie (1975) asked teachers ”What experiences do you think have been most influential in teaching you how to teach?" Only 15 percent of the respondents mentioned teacher education courses. Lortie concluded that ”formal pedagogical instruction had no greater valence than experiences in recreational work or motherhood" (p. T7). More recently, Smylie (1989) reported similar results from a national survey of approximately 1750 teachers. Of the fourteen items included on the survey, undergraduate education courses were rated thirteenth and regarded as one of the least effective sources of teachers' learning to teach; in fact, only district inservice training sessions were rated less effective. Like Lortie (1975), Smylie (1989) reported that beginning teachers felt as though their education 12 courses had not adequately prepared them for the classroom, and they criticized these courses as having been too theoretical and insufficiently practical. Numerous other researchers (e.g., Kagan, 1992; Koehler, 1985; Sarason, Davidson, & Blatt, 1986; Stone, 1987) have noted teachers' dissatisfaction with their education courses. Katz, Raths, Mohanty, Karachi, and Irving (cited in Stone, 1987) labeled beginning teachers' complaints about their preservice preparation as the ”feed-forward problem.” They argued that education courses often provide knowledge that the preservice teachers are not yet ready for and answers to questions that they have not yet asked, and then later in their actual teaching when they realize that they need this knowledge, the teachers complain that it was never presented. I would say, though, that whether or not teacher education programs are actually presenting prospective teachers with knowledge that is potentially useful, the fact remains that large numbers of teachers apparently feel as though they have gained little from their teacher education courses. But teachers themselves are not the only ones saying that education courses often have little impact on prospective teachers. The National Center for Research on Teacher Education (1991), in its final report on the Teacher Education and Learning to Teach (T ELT) study, commented that Compared with other influences on learning to teach, teacher education is generally regarded as a weak intervention. At the preservice level, this often means that programs do not challenge the "apprenticeship—of-obsei'vation' «the years of teacher-watching that shape prospective teachers' views of teaching. As a result, teachers often leave preservice preparation with their initial views intact. (p 67) Drawing from data collected in the TELT study, Paine (1989) examined the orientation towards diversity of 233 preservice teachers. She identified four different orientations to diversity: individual difference, where differences are viewed in terms of individuals' differing from each other in any number of respects (e.g., height, personality, intelligence); categorical difference, where differences are viewed as ”repeating patterns of variation across individuals' ( p. 3), including categories like social class, race, and gender; contextual difference, where "differences among individuals occur in patterns, 13 yet these patterns are seen as connected to a social situation or embedded in a larger, dynamic context” (p. 3); and pedagogical difference, where differences ”are understood as having pedagogical implications--consequences for both teaching and learning" (p. 3). Paine reported that most of the teacher candidates appeared to have an individual difference orientation to diversity; it was this orientation, as well as the categorical difference orientation, that they typically mentioned. She noted that the participants in this study reported having had little prior experience with diversity, having come from backgrounds that were quite homogeneous. Paine also observed that the prospective teachers seemed to place a great deal of importance on the ideas of equity and fairness; yet, at the same time, they expressed a lot of confusion and uncertainty about how they could achieve these conditions in their teaching. She concluded that ”these teachers bring approaches to diversity that have the potential for reproducing inequality and reflect larger social and historical dilemmas" (p. 20). However, it should be noted that the interviews in Paine’s study were conducted early in the students’ teacher education program and do not tell us anything about where these students may have ended up (L. Paine, personal communication, February 2, 1996). Although additional data about prospective teachers' views about diversity were collected in the TELT study, the researchers (National Center for Research on Teacher Education, 1991) explained that the instruments used to collect these data may have been flawed, and they cautioned, "we have reservations about the validity and reliability of these data” (p. 52). They went on to suggest, however, that in order to understand teachers' beliefs about teaching diverse learners, more attention would need to be given to the background experiences of the teachers. In one set of data the researchers observed that in an induction program the prospective teachers' beliefs about the sources of school failure seemed to move in the desired direction moreso than the beliefs of prospective teachers in other settings. The researchers hypothesized that The induction program, which seems most successful in influencing teachers' thinking about issues around teaching diverse learners, may have been influential 14 precisely because teachers were predisposed at the outset to think in the direction the program seemed to be directing them. (p. 60) However, since only very limited data were presented in the text of the report, it is not clear to me what in the data led the researchers to posit this particular hypothesis. Kagan (1992) reviewed 40 research studies on leaming-to-teach that were published between 1987 and 1991. She found that ”all but one study indicated that personal beliefs remained stable” (p. 156). Although Kagan uses terms like beliefs, attitudes, and images, she does not define any of them; however, her use of beliefs and attitudes in context seems consistent with the definitions I provided earlier. In one of the studies reviewed by Kagan, Weinstein (1990) assessed the impact of an introductory education course and field experience on prospective teachers' preconceptions about teaching. At the end of the semester she found virtually no change in their preconceptions. Weinstein argued that, though her study was only one semester in length, it was not unrealistic to expect that their preconceptions would have been affected by the course. It is not unreasonable to anticipate that students' expectations about their future teaching performance and their beliefs about good teaching would change as a result of an initial field experience and a course that stressed the complexity of classrooms and the many roles of teachers. (p. 285) In sum, the available research suggests that teacher education programs do not have a very powerful impact on prospective teachers’ beliefs and attitudes Influence of Students' Attitudes and Beliefs on Their Learning Not only are the entering perspectives of preservice teachers resistant to change, but also there now seems to be a general consensus among researchers that preservice teachers' entering beliefs and attitudes are major determinants of what they do and do not learn in their preparation programs. As Lortie (1975) explained, "one's personal dispositions are not only relevant but, in fact, stand at the core of becoming a teacher" (p. 79). Lortie does not define his use of "personal dispositions,” but it seems to be consistent with my definition of attitudes. Feiman-Nemser and Buchmann (1985) 15 conducted case studies of two prospective teachers during the first year of their preparation program and concluded that ”in becoming a teacher, very little normatively correct learning can be trusted to come about without instruction that takes the preconceptions of future teachers into account" (p. 29). Goodman (1988), who conducted an ethnographic study of students in an elementary education program, contended that preservice teachers' entering perspectives serve as "intuitive screens” through which they interpreted the content and experiences of their preparation program. He defined the term perspective as including ”an individual's actions rather than just his/her disposition to act" (p. 121). In other words, the term seems to represent a combination of attitude and behavior, but whether or not it is also intended to include beliefs is unclear, though I would assume so. Goodman found that the students tended to reject ideas and experiences that directly contradicted their "intuitive screens." From his study Goodman concluded, . . . it would appear that getting to know students' "intuitive screens” is extremely important, if educators are interested in having a significant impact upon their students. . . . Once the intuitive screen of a given student is understood by faculty members, then different strategies can be developed to best educate that individual. For some students, the faculty will want to offer experiences that will reinforce a given student‘s intuitive screen while in another instance, they will want to find ways to help a student alter his or her initial perspective. (p. 134) In her study of 14 teacher education students, Hollingsworth (1989) posited a notion similar to Goodman's intuitive screens when she said that 'preprogram beliefs served as filters for processing program content and making sense of classroom contexts” (p. 168). Feiman-Nemser and Buchmann (1989) followed six students through two years of their undergraduate teacher education program. Reinforcing the conclusion that they had reached in their earlier study, these researchers stressed the necessity of attending to the incoming beliefs and attitudes of prospective teachers. Feiman-Nemser and Buchmann went on to say, Teacher candidates are not blank slates; what they learn during teacher preparation is a function of what they bring to their professional studies as well as what they encounter in courses and field experiences. (pp. 375-376) 16 Numerous other researchers (e. g., Anderson & Holt-Reynolds, 1995; Ball, 1989; Bullough, 1991: Holt-Reynolds, 1991a; McDiarmid & Price, 1990; Pajares, 1992; Weinstein, 1990) have also concluded that if teacher educators are to be effective in helping prospective teachers reach desired knowledge and understandings, then the perspectives which they bring to their preparation program must be taken into account. Researchers at the National Center for Research on Teacher Education (1991) offered the following observation: There is now agreement that new knowledge is influenced by old knowledge, that existing conceptions and expectations significantly shape learning. It follows that how teachers think about leaming to teach will influence their openness and receptivity to different kinds of knowledge and sources of learning. (p. 69) Furthermore, several reviewers of the learning-to-teach literature have all reached essentially the same conclusion, lending it even further weight. Kagan (1992), for example, reviewed the literature on teacher growth, and one of her conclusions was The personal beliefs and images that preservice candidates bring to programs of teacher education usually remain inflexible. Candidates tend to use the information provided in course work to confirm rather than to confront and correct their preexisting beliefs. Thus, a candidate's personal beliefs and images determine how much knowledge the candidate acquires from a preservice program and how it is interpreted. (p. 154) Similarly, reviewing the literature on teachers' attitudes and beliefs, Richardson (1996) concluded, Teacher attitudes and beliefs, therefore, are important considerations in understanding classroom practices and conducting teacher education designed to help prospective and inservice teachers develop their thinking and practices. In such change programs, beliefs and attitudes of incoming preservice students and inservice teachers strongly affect what and how they learn and are also targets of change within the process. (p. 1) Richardson discussed some of the various meanings that have been attributed to attitudes, but she never specified which definition she would be using in her chapter. The last definition that she mentioned was by Fishbein, who described attitudes as "learned predispositions to respond to an object or class of objects in a favorable or unfavorable way” (p. 3). This definition is consistent with how I have defined the term. Richardson 17 defines belief as "a proposition that is accepted as true by the individual holding the belief” (pp. 5-6). This definition is also consistent with mine. In their review of the literature on learning to teach, Borko and Putnam (in press) made an observation that was almost identical to Richardson's: ”research on learning to teach shows that teachers‘ existing knowledge and beliefs are critical in shaping what and how they learn from teacher education experiences” (p. 5). Unfortunately, these reviewers did not define what they meant by beliefs, but their use of the term does seem consistent with my earlier definition. Later in their chapter, referring to research conducted by Ross, Johnson and Smith at the University of Florida, Borko and Putnam go on to say, . . . learning to teach is influenced by a complex array of factors, one of the most significant of which is the entering perspective of the student. Entering perspective serves as a filter that determines how experiences within the teacher education program are interpreted. (p. 15) Some of the research on multicultural teacher education courses has also commented on the importance of prospective teachers' beliefs. Studying a group of teacher candidates in a course on diversity, McGeehan (1982) found that "participants displaying the greatest gains in knowledge of multicultural education tended to express more accepting initial attitudes and feelings toward other ethnic groups” (p. 166). In other words, students who began the course with more positive attitudes toward other ethnic groups appeared to learn more from the course. McDiarmid and Price (1990) examined the effects of a multicultural training program on a group of prospective teachers, and they found that there was little change in the teachers' belief at the end of the training. The researchers speculated that in order for desired changes to occur, some of the incoming beliefs of prospective teachers may need to be directly challenged. One reason that prospective teachers' entering beliefs are so powerful is due to what Lortie (1975) has termed the "apprenticeship of observation.” Prospective teachers have all spent thousands of hours in classrooms and enter their teacher education programs with many solidly-entrenched beliefs about teaching and learning. Holt- Reynolds (1991b) explained that teacher candidates tend to use their "personal 18 history-based beliefs" as a measuring stick against which they test the validity and utility of the new information that they are presented with in their teacher education courses. Or, put another way, new information is filtered through the beliefs that they have already formed through their personal experiences, and these prior beliefs serve both to help and hinder the individual's learning from their program. Holt-Reynolds illustrated this process by describing how nine preservice teachers' entering perspective of teaching as being teacher-centered led them to reject one of the central messages of a content-area reading course in which the instructor had stressed the importance of student-centered instruction. Instead of questioning their entering perspective, these students instead questioned the instructor's message, evaluating it on the basis of their personal history- based beliefs about the teachers' role. Consequently, what these students learned from the course was different than what the instructor had intended. Holt-Reynolds (1991a) has argued that, to be more effective in our task of educating prospective teachers, "it is incumbent upon us to enter the dialogues of our students of teaching and work with their assumptions, acknowledging them before we try to reshape them'I (p. 20). Richardson (1996) seemed to reach a similar conclusion: Studies of the origins of teachers' beliefs indicate that many different life experiences contribute to the formation of strong and enduring beliefs about teaching and learning. Within a constructivist learning and teaching framework, these beliefs should be surfaced and acknowledged during the teacher education program if the program is to make a difference in the deep structure of knowledge and beliefs held by the students. (p. 11) To say that prospective teachers' entering attitudes and beliefs influence what they learn from their preparation programs is not necessarily to say that they always learn little or that their beliefs and attitudes are never changed. Some researchers have reported success in changing their students' incoming beliefs. Noordhoff and Kleinfeld (1993), for example, investigated the impact of the Teachers for Alaska (TFA) program and reported ”substantial growth in our prospective teachers' orientations toward teaching culturally diverse students” (p. 35). The researchers attribute the success of the program to its strong field-experience components and the emphasis on students' being reflective 19 and developing understandings of the particular contexts in which they were teaching. However, Noordhof f and Kleinfeld also acknowledged that the success may be partially attributable to the selectivity of the program, in that only students with certain characteristics may have chosen that sort of program. Bondy, Schmitz, and Johnson (1993) investigated the influence of a combination of course work and field experience on prospective teachers' beliefs about teaching minority students. They found that field experience, in combination with course work which allowed them to process and make sense of those experiences, led to positive changes in prospective teachers' beliefs about teaching poor and minority students. In contrast, field experience by itself actually seemed to be counterproductive. Anderson and Holt-Reynolds (1995) studied how a content-area reading instructor (i.e., Holt-Reynolds) attempted to take into consideration in her instruction the incoming beliefs of her students. The instructor first hypothesized about which of her prospective teachers' incoming beliefs might influence their learning from the course, and then during the course she employed a number of pedagogical moves intended to challenge those beliefs. The researchers found that by the end of the course, two of the three case study students had transformed their beliefs in the desired direction. The beliefs of the third student did not change, however, presumably because his incoming beliefs were somewhat different than what the instructor had anticipated and were not directly challenged by her pedagogical moves. Despite the very small sample, the results of this study suggest that when prospective teachers' incoming beliefs are appropriately surfaced and challenged, then changing those beliefs may be more likely and more of the desired learning is likely to occur. In summary, the fact that prospective teachers' attitudes and beliefs influence what and how much they learn from their preparation program appears to be well-documented in the learning-to-teach literature. Some of the research discussed in this section also suggests that prospective teachers are likely to accept new information which is consistent with their existing beliefs, but they tend to be resistant to information which 20 conflicts with their beliefs. In other words, prospective teachers' entering beliefs about teaching appear to strongly influence how they respond to information about teaching that is presented to them in their education courses. For me, these findings raise the question of whether or not students' existing racial attitudes and beliefs might, in a similar manner, mediate what they learn about racial minority groups from courses on diversity. Just as prospective teachers' entering beliefs about teaching serve as filters through which they perceive the information about teaching that is presented in their preparation program, their racial attitudes and beliefs might serve to filter their perceptions of information about racial minorities. To explore this possibility further, it makes sense to turn to the research on racial attitudes and beliefs. Racial Attitudes and Beliefs In this section I will discuss how people's attitudes and beliefs influence their perception of new information, how negative racial attitudes are still a widespread problem in this country, and how racial attitudes influence people's behavior toward racial minorities. I uence on rce tion 0 New Information There is a considerable body of research indicating that both beliefs and attitudes are important influences on how people interpret information and situations that they are presented with (Bar-Ta], 1990; Bern, 1970; Oskamp, 1991; Petty & Cacioppo, 1981; Scheibe, 1970; Zimbardo & Leippe, 1991). Bar-Ta] (1990) explained that knowledge formation is always dependent on previously-acquired beliefs, which determine how new information will be perceived, interpreted, and evaluated. According to Zimbardo and Leippe (1991), this filtering process is not always logical nor objective. Human minds select and interpret information to make it fit with established beliefs and attitudes. . . . People are "cognitive conservatives” in that they resist changing their thoughts and evaluations of the objects of their world. . . . Despite good intentions to be objective, people tend to gather information in such a way as to stack the deck in favor of confirming their beliefs or "working hypotheses” about an object. (pp. 204205) 21 Zimbardo and Leippe further noted that beliefs, once formed, are quite resistant to change. People will often cling to a belief even in the face of substantial disconfirrnatory evidence. In his review of the literature on teachers' beliefs, Pajares (1992) reported similar conclusions. Because teachers' beliefs serve as filters through which new information is interpreted, Pajares suggested that "the investigation of teachers' beliefs is a necessary and valuable avenue of educational inquiry” (p. 326). Some research indicates that attitudes also influence the acquisition and interpretation of new knowledge. Consistency theorists (see Oskamp, 1991; Petty & Cacioppo, 1981; Triandis, 1971) claim that people attempt to maintain consistency between their attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors. When they are confronted with new information which is inconsistent with their current perspectives, then attitude change is possible as they endeavor to maintain consistency. However, much as is the case with their beliefs, people tend to interpret new information in a way that is consistent with their existing attitudes. Consistency theorists believe that people's desire for consistency leads them to ”such 'illogical' ways of maintaining consistency as denial of the truth of the new information which conflicts with a person's present viewpoints, or searching for supportive data to bolster present attitudes when they have been challenged by new information” (Oskamp, 1991, p. 231). People are more likely to perceive as logical and valid arguments that are in agreement with their existing attitudes (Bethlehem, 1985). Zimbardo and Leippe (1991) have pointed out that people's attitudes can color their perception of new information even in areas about which they possess little prior knowledge. We can have an attitude about something--liking or disliking itneven when the rest of the mental representation is practically devoid of beliefs and actual knowledge. This is true of many of our prejudices-the negative attitudes that we may develop about groups of people that we actually know little about. . . . attitudes which have little or no basis in knowledge may subsequently affect the acquisition of knowledge and the formation of beliefs that may eventually 'fill" the formerly empty mental representation. Our overall evaluation of something affects how we interpret what we read and hear about the object. (pp. 34-35) 22 Thus, there seems to be clear evidence that people's existing attitudes and beliefs have a definite influence on how they interpret new information. In the previous section of this chapter I discussed research indicating that preservice teachers' entering perspectives on teaching appear to mediate what they learn from their teacher education courses. It seems reasonable to assume that, in a similar fashion, preservice teachers' entering attitudes toward and beliefs about racial minorities are likely to mediate what they learn from courses on diversity. This circumstance, if true, is a cause for concern because there is substantial empirical evidence suggesting that many White Americans still harbor negative racial attitudes and beliefs. The Persistence of Negative Racial Attitudes From its earliest beginnings, American society has been and continues to be plagued by the specter of racism (Bennett, 1987; Schwartz & Disch, 1970). In 1968 the National Advisory Commission on Civil Disorders, assigned to identify the causes underlying the recurring racial disorders in American cities, reported: the causes of the recent racial disorders are embedded in a massive tangle of issues and circumstances-social, economic, political, and psychological--which arise out of the historical pattern of Negro-white relations in America. These factors are both complex and interacting. . . . Despite these complexities, certain fundamental matters are clear. Of these, the most fundamental is the racial attitude and behavior of white Americans toward black Americans. Race prejudice has shaped our history decisively in the past; it now threatens to do so again. White racism is essentially responsible for the explosive mixture which has been accumulating in our cities since the end of World War II. (p. 203) Twenty years later, a comprehensive study of the status of Black Americans (Jaynes & Williams, 1989) revealed that, despite definite progress in many areas, the problem of racism persisted. Foremost among the reasons for the present state of black-white relations are two continuing consequences of the nation's long and recent history of racial inequality. One is the negative attitudes held toward blacks and the other is the actual disadvantaged conditions under which many black Americans live. These two consequences reinforce each other. Thus, a legacy of discrimination and segregation continues to affect black-white relations." (p. 5) During the past six years, racial disorders in cities like Los Angeles, Miami, New York, and Boston, along with a resurgence of racial incidents on college campuses, amply attest 23 to the continuing existence of the problem. Negative racial attitudes and stereotypes continue to be widespread in today's society. According to Ponterotto and Pedersen (1993), ”The view of many social scientists is that you cannot be socialized into a society stratified along racial lines and not be influenced by racist ideology” (pp. 18-19). Racial prejudices are learned, starting as early as age 3 or 4, from parents, family, peers, and other larger community influences (Barndt, 1991; Jones, 1972; Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993). Moore (1984) argued that the media, which is characterized by "a profound white bias that distorts our perspectives and our children's” (p. 40), is a significant contributor to the racist socialization and acculturation process. Although it is abundantly clear that racism still persists in our society, a series of national polls also indicate that since 1942 White attitudes toward African Americans have become increasingly more positive (Dovido & Gaertner, 1991; Myers, 1990). This contradictory trend is misleading, however, for at least three reasons. First, overt expressions of racism are no longer considered socially desirable in most contexts, and some evidence (Duckitt, 1992; Kleg, 1993) indicates that questionnaire respondents often report being more racially tolerant than they actually are. As Maluso (1995) explained, "Contemporary theorists almost unanimously adhere to the position that the salience of the cultural norm of egalitarianism will inhibit expressions of racism” (p. 54). In other words, individuals are less likely to display prejudiced attitudes or behaviors in situations where such attitudes and behaviors would be disapproved. For example, Frey and Gaertner (cited in Maluso, 1995) found that "130 White female university students only refused to help Black recipients when the failure to help would not be perceived by others as socially inappropriate” (p. 55). Also, according to Maluso, expressions of racism by White respondents seem to be inhibited in the presence of Black researchers and in situations where their responses to questionnaire items were not anonymous. Dovido and Gaertrrer (1991), suggesting that White racial attitudes may actually have changed little since 1964, note that ”feelings toward blacks, as measured by the cold-warm loo-point 24 thermometer scale of the Institute for Social Research (ISR), has remained remarkably stable” (p. 126). Second, the apparent improvement in Whites' attitudes toward Blacks is further called into question by a recent study of Blacks in American society. The researchers (Jaynes & Williams, 1989) reported, "The foremost conclusion is that race still matters greatly in the United States. Much of the evidence reviewed in this report indicates widespread attitudes of racism" (p. 155) If polls show that Whites' racial attitudes are improving, how can racism still be widespread? Ponterotto and Pedersen (1993) have offered one explanation for why this racism doesn't show up in recent self -reports of racial attitudes: One major theme pervading the newer conceptions of prejudice is that racist attitudes and beliefs are outside conscious awareness. Given that racist views contradict most people's stated value system, one's view of self would be impacted negatively if a racist ideology were acknowledged. Therefore, holding racist beliefs out of conscious awareness keeps the self concept intact. (p. 18) Additionally, as Maluso (1995) observed, ”participants tend to underreport adherence to racial stereotypes in order to appear socially acceptable” (p. 56). The fact that people tend to give socially desirable responses and underreport their racial biases is important because it suggests that negative racial attitudes may be more prevalent than is generally assumed. Another explanation for this seeming contradiction is that the nature of racism in this country has changed. Some researchers (see Duckitt, I992; Maluso, 1995; Oskamp, 1991; Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993) contend that old-style, overt racism has declined, but a new form of racism, sometimes called modern racism or symbolic racism, is thriving and may actually be increasing. Kinder and Sears (cited in Duckitt, 1992) describe symbolic racism as " a form of resistance to change in the racial status quo based on moral feelings that blacks violate such traditional American values as individualism and self ~reliance, the work ethic, obedience, and discipline” (pp. 19-20). Negative racial feelings that were once expressed overtly by Whites are now being expressed covertly 25 through their opposition to various issues often associated with minorities, such as affirmative action, welfare, and busing. Oskarnp (1991) explained that whites generally do not perceive their attitudes on these new issues as racist, and they tend to justify their opinions on nonracial grounds. Examples of such new issues include beliefs as to whether there is continuing racial discrimination in US. society, attitudes toward busing of school children, and support for various government programs to aid the poor. (p. 383) It should be noted, however, that not all scholars agree with the concept of symbolic racism nor with the idea that it is a new form of racism. Sniderrnan and Tetlock (1986), for example, while not disputing that expressions of racism might change over time, have questioned whether it is reasonable to assume that people who take political positions in opposition to certain issues are necessarily racists. In addition, Sniderman and Tetlock have argued that research on symbolic racism suggests that, despite the claims of its major proponents, it is not truly a new form of racism; rather, it is traditional racism expressed in different ways. Investigating the question of whether symbolic racism was fundamentally different from traditional racism, Snidennan and Tetlock (cited in Duckitt, 1992) determined that, "it appears that every major cause of symbolic racism is, simultaneously, a major cause of old-fashioned racism; and every major cause of old- fashioned racism is a major cause of symbolic [racism]" (p.22). Duckitt also reported that researchers have found consistent strong correlations between scales used to measure traditional racism and those used to measure symbolic racism. He concluded that symbolic racism appears to be ”simply a more subtle, more socially acceptable, more sophisticated, and more covert expression of racial prejudice" (p. 22). Despite the controversy surrounding the concept of symbolic racism, having an awareness that racism today tends to be expressed more subtly is important because individuals--including teacher educators--who are not aware of this modern form of racism and who see few overt expressions of prejudice-might mistakenly conclude that racial prejudice is no longer a problem when, in fact, it actually is. 26 A final reason polls showing improved attitudes are misleading is that they assess the attitudes only of individuals, not of institutions. Like individual racism, institutional racism in today's society is less overt than in the past, but it is, nonetheless, still deeply woven into the fabric of American life (Bamdt, 1991). Jones (1972) defined institutional racism as . . .those established laws, customs, and practices which systematically reflect and produce racial inequities in American society. If racist consequences accrue to institutional laws, customs, or practices, the institution is racist whether or not the individuals maintaining those practices have racist intentions. (p. 131, emphasis in original) In other words, even though individuals may not consider themselves to be racist, their actions (or inaction) serve to perpetuate racist practices in society's institutions, including schools, where standardized tests, tracking, special education referrals, discipline policies, and other insidiously biased practices militate against the success of minority children (Moore, 1984). Bamdt (1991) has argued that it is easy for the vast majority of Whites to accept society's institutions and practices as they are because they benefit from institutional racism. Oskarnp (1991) observed that institutional racism and individual racism reinforce each other“. "It is doubtful that institutional racism could develop or survive without support from many individual racists, nor is it likely that individual racism would thrive without strong social support" (p. 374). This suggests that one way to combat institutional racism may be to raise individuals' awareness of it and of their own racial attitudes. Influence on Behavior toward Racial Minorities lndividuals' racial attitudes and beliefs influence not only how they interpret new information, but also how they behave toward racial minorities. Although research (Maluso, 1995; Myers, 1990; Oskamp, 1991; Sampson, 1991) indicates that people's attitudes are not reliable predictors of their behavior, an attitude is more than just a harmless feeling. Jackrnan (cited in Lynch, 1987) has suggested that negative beliefs or stereotypes about a group predisposes people to act in certain ways toward them. Duckitt 27 (1992), reviewing research on the relationship between attitudes and behavior, asserted that the relationship between the two may be stronger than has generally been assumed. Maluso (1995), referring to a literature review by Crosby, Bromley, and Saxe, observed that ”unobtrusive studies of interpersonal racism show that anti-Black behaviors continue to occur even though self -reports indicate that interpersonal racist attitudes have decreased” (p. 56). She also cited several research studies which illustrated that White subjects treated Black individuals differently than they did White individuals in the same situation. For example, when a Black person and a White person each demonstrated pushing or shoving behavior, the Black person was typically judged to be more aggressive or more hostile than the White person. Bond, DiCandia, and MacKinnon (cited in Maluso, 1995) found that among adolescent patients in a psychiatric hospital, "although there was no difference in the number of violent acts performed by White and Black patients, Blacks were four times more likely to be physically restrained than Whites' (p. 54). Among other studies cited by Maluso, one documented that White customers avoided a Black salesclerk, another revealed that White interviewers sat farther away from Black interviewees than from White interviewees, and a third found that White women, given a choice of seats, tended to sit farther away from Blacks than from Whites. Additionally, Maluso identified research which had found that Black students . were suspended from school more frequently than White students, Black prison inmates received longer sentences than did White inmates for the same crimes, and at community health centers Black patients were more likely than White patients to be treated by paraprofessionals. This research evidence strongly suggests that negative racial attitudes and stereotypes are still manifesting themselves in differential treatment for racial minorities. In summary, research indicates that people's beliefs and attitudes strongly influence how new information is interpreted. Beliefs and attitudes are typically quite resistant to change because of people's tendency to interpret new information in ways that 28 are consistent with their existing conceptions and feelings, even if doing so means having to distort or even deny the information. The research further indicates that while White Americans generally report greater acceptance of racial minorities, negative racial attitudes and beliefs are still a problem in American society and still influence how White Americans treat racial minorities in both educational and non-educational settings, albeit in more subtle ways than in the past. These research findings are important because it has been projected that during the next 25 years, the teaching force in this country will remain predominantly White, while the student population will become more racially diverse (Pallas, Natriello, and McDill, 1989). In light of these changing demographic trends, the racial attitudes and beliefs of these White teachers become very important, because if they are negative, then the research cited in this section suggests that these teachers, like White Americans in the general population, are likely to respond to racial minority students differently than they respond to majority students. Given that prospective teachers' beginning beliefs act as filters and given that their racial beliefs in particular may lead to differential treatment for many minority students, I would contend that these beliefs need to be addressed in multicultural teacher education. While many teacher educators are presently calling for greater emphasis on multicultural teacher education, some of their underlying assumptions, as I will explain in the next section, need to be reexamined, and more attention needs to be given to the potential influence of negative racial attitudes. Multicultural Teacher Education In this section I will discuss some of the different conceptions about the need for multicultural teacher education and about the strategies for providing it. Thus far, there appears to have been much more rhetoric than research offered in support of the current push for teacher education for diversity. In their review of the literature on preparing teachers for diversity, Grant and Secada (1990) reported finding few empirical studies of the subject. They concluded that this paucity of research ”suggests the marginal status 29 and low importance that has been given to research on the preparation of teachers to work with diverse student populations" (p. 404). Zeichner (1993) also noted that this topic has received ”relatively very little attention" in the literature, and he agreed with Grant and Secada that the issue of how to prepare teachers to work with diverse learners apparently has low status in the agenda for teacher education reform. The literature on multicultural teacher education is not only lacking in empirical evidence, but is also plagued with numerous unexamined assumptions and unanswered questions, including: Why are the changing demographic trends problematic? What does it mean to prepare teachers for diversity? These two questions will serve to frame my review of the literature on preparing teachers for working with diverse learners. I will argue that the changing demographics are problematic not because of prospective teachers' lack of knowledge about minorities, as seems to be generally assumed, but because of the probability that many of these teachers are likely to bring with them negative racial attitudes and beliefs which will negatively impact their learning from courses on diversity as well as their ability to work effectively with minority children. Why Are the Changing Demographic Trends Problematic? The disturbing demographic trends in this country are mentioned frequently in the literature, and most writers have asserted, explicitly or implicitly, that having a predominantly White and female teaching force working with an increasingly diverse student population is undesirable, if not problematic. However, few writers have offered much in the way of evidence to substantiate their contention. Although I agree with their assumption, like Grant and Secada (1990) I also believe that "agreement should not preclude empirical inquiry about that assumption" (p. 406). What makes this situation so bad? While advocates very rarely address this important question directly, the solutions that they are proposing offer clues to their conceptions of why the changing demographic trends may be considered problematic. Most of the proposals for multicultural teacher education seem to suggest explanations that fall into one of three broad categories: 30 potential cultural mismatches, different learning styles, and negative teacher attitudes toward culturally/ethnically different students. Central to each of these explanations is a particular conception of what prospective teachers need to develop or how they need to change in order to be prepared to work with diverse learners. In the first two explanations additional knowledge is considered to be the primary need, while the third explanation specifies the need to develop positive racial attitudes. In the following pages I will describe these three perspectives and identify their major weaknesses. In so doing, I will argue that the high probability of negative teacher attitudes toward minority students is an important and understudied feature of the changing demographic trends. Cultural Mismatches That the cultural mismatch between teachers and their students will result in lower achievement for some students is one of the most commonly offered explanations of the need for teacher education for diversity. As Au and Kawakarni (1994) explained, "The overall hypothesis in research on cultural congruence is that students of diverse backgrounds often do poorly in school because of a mismatch between the culture of the school and the culture of the home” (pp. 5-6). Because the demographic projections indicate that more and more students will come from cultural backgrounds different from those of their teachers, the problem of cultural incongruence is likely to increase unless prospective teachers are made aware of and prepared to deal with the incongruities. Numerous researchers (e.g., Burstein & Cabello, 1989; Davidman, 1990; Irvine, 1992; Larke, 1990) have illustrated how cultural mismatches can result in frustration for students and teachers alike and also have suggested that educators who demonstrate higher levels of cultural sensitivity and knowledge are more effective with minority learners than are educators who demonstrate lower levels. Specifically, the work of Au (1980) with tum-taking among Hawaiian children during reading instruction, Heath (1982) with the questioning strategies of Black children in Trackton, and Michaels (cited in Villegas, 1988) with the narrative styles of Black children in a first grade classroom are 31 sometimes cited by proponents of the cultural congruence explanation as examples of how recognizing the differences between the culture of the home and the culture of the school and then responding appropriately to them can lead to more success for minority children. While the aforementioned research has focused on cultural mismatches between the school and the home, other research which I would also classify in this category has been concerned primarily with differences in the cultural or racial backgrounds of individual teachers and students. Noordhof f and Kleinfeld (1993) argued that when teachers' cultural backgrounds differ from those of their students, it appears to be more difficult for teachers to accurately interpret student behavior and classroom situations and also to make appropriate teaching decisions. The authors concluded that "when teachers and students have little or no shared background, students' opportunities to learn worthwhile knowledge are at risk" (p. 28); therefore, they contended that prospective teachers need to learn about the particular students whom they will be teaching. Some educators have expressed some grave doubts about the utility of the cultural congruence hypothesis. Though not speaking specifically about teacher education, Gibson (1984) criticized this approach because "it accepts without question that cultural differences are the cause of minority groups' failure in mainstream schools, and it assumes that multicultural education is a viable solution to the problem of past failure” (p. 97). She argued that despite its avowed emphasis on cultural differences (as opposed to deficits), in practice it would still represent a means of accommodating (compensating for) the special needs (deficits) of special groups of children because it is unlikely that the norms of minority cultural groups would ever achieve parity with the norms of the majority culture. In other words, it is likely that the culture of the school (i.e., the majority culture) will continue to be the yardstick by which the culture of the home (i.e., minority culture) is measured. Gibson chose to label this approach as “benevolent multiculturalism,“ regarding it as paternalistic and patronizing. She quotes Paulo Freire, 32 who wrote, "Pedagogy, which begins with the egoistic interests of the oppressors (an egoism cloaked in the false generosity of paternalism) and makes of the oppressed the objects of its humanitarianism, itself maintains and embodies oppression” (p. 98). McDiarmid (1992) has also questioned the efficacy of the cultural difference approach to teacher education. He has pointed out that ”little agreement exists on which differences matter, how they matter, and how teachers should address or accommodate these in the classroom” (p. 84). After analyzing data from a four-year longitudinal study of a multicultural training program which presented beginning teachers with information on diverse learners, McDiarmid noticed no apparent improvement in the academic performance of minority students. It is possible, of course, that the observed lack of improvement in minority achievement was attributable to inadequacies in the delivery of the multicultural instruction rather than to any inadequacy in the cultural difference hypothesis; however, McDiarmid also reasoned that ' If the problem were principally a matter of teachers' knowledge of culturally different students' backgrounds, we should expect that minority students would do significantly and consistently better in the classrooms of teachers who share their backgrounds. Yet, evidence does not support such a conclusion. (p. 89) This statement suggests that this hypothesis may offer an inadequate explanation of the problem. Finally, numerous writers (e.g., Cazden & Mehan, 1989; Florio-Ruane, 1994; Garibaldi, 1993; Noordhoff & Kleinfeld, 1993; Zimper & Ashbum, 1993) have warned that teacher educators must be cautious about teaching students generalizations about various racial/cultural groups. McDiarmid and Price (1990) found that "the presentation of information on ethnic and religious groups may actually encourage prospective teachers to generalize and, eventually, to prejudge pupils in their classrooms" (p. 15). McDiarmid and Price (1990) and Noordhoff and Kleinfeld (1993) have suggested that prospective teachers be given opportunities to learn about specific children and their farme background and experiences. Such an approach might lessen their tendencies to develop stereotypes about entire groups. According to Gibson (1984), another danger of 33 teaching generalizations is that it may lead prospective teachers to think of racial/cultural groups as each being a uniform whole, possessing clearly discernible traits, whereas in reality tremendous variations exist within any such group. Gibson has also questioned the common tendency of equating cultural groups with ethnic groups. She explained, Traditionally, anthropologists--and hence educators-have associated culture only with relatively self -suf ficient social groups, rather than with sets of people who ”repeatedly participate with one another in one or more activities.” But just as individuals have varying degrees of competence in varying numbers of dialects or languages, and varying understandings of the situations in which each rs appropriate, so, too, individuals have varying degrees of competence in varying numbers of cultures. (p. 112) In other words, each person is a member of and is influenced by multiple cultural groups, many of which cut across racial/ethnic lines, and it would be inaccurate and misleading to suggest that students' racial/ethnic group membership is always the strongest or most consistent determinant of their behavior. China and Plata (1987/88) observed that ”ethnicity is only one of many significant, interdependent variables which contribute to the development of students' behavioral patterns" (p. 9), and Grant and Sleeter (1986) have also expressed concern about researchers' tendency to regard racial groups as though they were homogeneous. While their discussion focused on the interaction of race, class, and gender, Grant and Sleeter argued that all people are members of multiple groups and that ”these simultaneous memberships influence perceptions and actions“ (p. 196). They cautioned that "A failure to consider the integration of race, social class, and gender leads at times to an oversimplification or inaccurate understanding of what occurs in schools, and therefore to inappropriate or simplistic prescriptions for educational equity" (p. 197). It would seem, therefore, that even though the cultural congruence hypothesis has shown promise in some research, some researchers seriously question its utility as an approach to multicultural teacher education. ' erent ' les The second explanation as to why the changing demographic trends are problematic is closely related to the first explanation: students of different cultural 34 groups may have different learning styles than majority students and, therefore, may respond better to different teaching styles (see Saracho & Gerstl, 1991; Shade, 1989c). According to proponents of this explanation, the teaching styles that prospective teachers learn in teacher preparation programs are best suited for majority students and do not match the learning styles of minority students, and the result is lower minority achievement. This explanation seems to presuppose that prospective teachers need to possess pedagogical knowledge and skills that are somehow different from those necessary for teaching majority students. Essentially, learning style refers to a person's preferred way of learning, the manner in which he/she most characteristically receives, processes, and remembers information (Holland, 1989). (Although More [1989] pointed out that learning style refers to a more general set of processes than does cognitive style. , the two terms are commonly used almost interchangeably in the literature and will be used in like manner in this discussion.) Various types of learning styles have been identified (Shade, 1989a), though the most researched and best known are the field dependent (relational) and the field independent (analytical) styles (see Table 2.1). Some research (e.g., Hale, 1982; Saracho & Gerstl, 1991; Shade, 1989b) suggests that cultural differences in child-rearing and socialization practices result in the development of different learning styles among children of minority groups. For example, there is evidence that African American children develop more of a relational learning style, whereas middle-class, White children tend to develop an analytical learning style. A problem arises because the educational system in this country seems best suited for children with analytical learning styles, and it fails to accommodate those who learn differently, as Irvine (1992) explains, Differences in cognitive style do not imply a superiority or inferiority relationship between analytical and relational style. Nor does it argue that all black children possess a relational style. However, when teachers use only analytical teaching methodologies and ignore relational methods of instruction, they fail to capitalize on the respective strengths of children's different learning modalities. (p. 86) 35 Table 2.1 Characteristics of Field Dependence and Field Independence Field-demndent Individuals Field-indemndent Individuals rely on the surrounding perceptual field perceive objects as separate from the field experience their environment in a relatively global can abstract an item from the surrounding field fashion by conforming to the effects of the prevailing field or context solve problems that are presented and reorganized in different contexts are dependent on authority experience an independence from authority which searchforfacialcuesinthosearoundthemasa leadsthemtodependontheirownstandardsand source of information values are strongly interested in people are oriented toward active striving getclosertothepersonwithwhomtheyare appeartobecoldanddistant interacting are socially detached but have analytic skills have a sensitivity to others which helps them to acquire social skills prefer occupations that allow them to work by themselves prefer occupations which require involvement with others --taken from Saracho and Gerstl, 1991»- More research has been done on African Americans than on other minority groups, but investigators (e.g., More, 1989; Pepper & Henry, 1989; Saracho, 1989; Saracho & Gerstl, 1991) have reported on "culturally induced" cognitive styles for Hispanics, Native Americans, and Asian Americans, although studies of the latter group have been very limited. The criticisms of the learning styles explanation are very much like those presented for the cultural mismatch explanation. One criticism of the cultural difference explanation was that there was a lack of evidence that adopting this approach produced higher student achievement. Similarly, in reference to the different learning styles explanation, Ladson-Billings (1993) noted, there is little evidence to suggest that distinguishing students according to their learning styles makes any significant differences in their academic performance. Each of these areas requires further exploration before we can accept or reject the saliency of learning styles as a way of addressing the educational needs of students. (p. 108) 36 Secondly, Gibson's (1984) criticism of the cultural difference theory applies here as well: despite proponents' claims that no one learning style should be viewed as superior or preferable to another, this approach might still be viewed as compensatory, as a means of accommodating children with special needs. A third weakness of the learning styles explanation was set forth by Holland (1989): While matching learning styles to teaching styles appears to be desirable, it may be rather idealistic. Such a matching process assumes a neat correlation between the teachers' instructional abilities and the learners' needs. Further, learning styles are not consistent and may change with the demands of the specific instructional activity. (p. 179) Finally, the learning styles explanation, because it, too, deals with making generalizations about minority groups, has the same danger of reinforcing prospective teachers' stereotypes or encouraging new ones. Negative Racial Attitudes and Beliefs There is a third explanation of the need for multicultural teacher education which, in the perception of this researcher, is much more compelling than the previous two. As explained in the previous section of this chapter, negative racial attitudes and beliefs are still a problem in American society, and they still influence how some White Americans behave toward people of color. At a large midwestem university, Law and Lane (1987) assessed the attitudes of 87 White, middle-class preservice teachers toward minority groups and found that “teachers ready to enter the classroom are no more accepting of various ethnic groups than the national samples spanning six decades. In fact, of practical significance is that the current study reflects a trend of less acceptance” (p. 5). In other words, preservice teachers appear to be no more accepting-and may be even less accepting-moi minority groups today than they were sixty years ago, a time when racial prejudice was both more overtly expressed and, supposedly, more widespread. Shoop and Eads (cited in Law & Lane, 1987) reported that when teachers hold negative attitudes toward particular groups, students from those groups may be treated differently in the classroom. Herein might lie the central problem of the contrasting demographic trends: 37 that a largely White and female teaching force may bring negative, unaccepting attitudes toward the growing numbers of students of color in their classrooms; these attitudes, coupled with the attendant lower expectations, are major contributing factors to the widespread academic failure among minority students. Law and Lane concluded that their study indicates the necessity of providing preservice teachers with multicultural education as a means of combating their tendencies to have negative, unaccepting attitudes toward minority groups. In the previous section of this chapter, I summarized research which indicated that people's racial attitudes and beliefs influence both how they interpret new information about racial groups and how they are inclined to behave toward members of those groups. Additionally, I cited evidence indicating that negative racial attitudes and beliefs are still a problem in American society and that in many situations Blacks are treated differently than Whites. Given that racist attitudes and discriminatory actions are still so pervasive in American society as a whole, it is unreasonable to assume that White college students entering the teaching profession will be any less likely than other Whites to manifest these attitudes. When Bymes and Kiger (1988) administered two questionnaires to 301 non-Black college students, they found that the racial attitudes of teacher education students did not differ significantly from those of non-education students. Clearly, we are as likely to encounter negative racial attitudes among prospective teachers as among other college students, and teacher educators need to acknowledge the existence of these attitudes and attempt to address them. One criticism of the negative attitudes explanation is that focusing only on racial attitudes ignores the problems resulting from negative gender and social class attitudes. However, positing negative racial attitudes as the central problem does not mean that issues of class and gender must necessarily be ignored. Attention certainly could be accorded to each of these issues and to ways in which they interact with race. Another criticism is that individual teachers' attitudes are not the major impediment to the 38 achievement of minority students. Far bigger impediments are the long-established institutional and societal inequities. While this is true, unless prospective teachers are made aware of their own racial attitudes and also educated about the institutional and societal causes of lower minority achievement, they are not likely to have the awareness or the knowledge to challenge the status quo. Conclusions The three preceding explanations have two important similarities. The first is that each of them assumes that the appropriate response to the changing demographic trends in this country involves changing something in the minds or hearts of prospective teachers. Others have conceptualized the appropriate response differently (e. g., see Gillette & Grant, 1991; Trent, 1990), as calling for a broader societal response. Thus, the three explanations presented here are all vulnerable to the criticism which Villegas ((1988) directed at the cultural difference hypothesis: However appealing these culturally sensitive proposals may seem, they are seriously flawed in that they leave unexamined the social inequality underlying the problem... By ignoring the political nature of schooling and its relation to the dominant society, we help to perpetuate a system of inequality, thus reducing our chances of effecting the change we claim to seek. (p. 258) While acknowledging the central role that sociopolitical power and privilege have in the persistent academic difficulties of poor and minority children, I also believe that change efforts will need to be mounted at many different levels, and it seems only reasonable that teacher educators direct their efforts at changing prospective teachers. The second similarity is that each explanation suggests a particular conception of what it is that prospective teachers need in order to work effectively with diverse learners. Both the cultural difference explanation and the different learning styles explanation have as their major emphasis the prospective teachers' acquisition of knowledge-either knowledge of other cultural groups or knowledge of different learning styles. In contrast, the major emphasis in the negative attitudes explanation is on developing positive attitudes toward different cultural groups. These different emphases would 39 seem to dictate the use of different instructional strategies, which I will discuss in the final section of this chapter What Does It Mean to Prepare Teariers for Diverw While there seems to be general agreement among most teacher educators about the need to prepare teachers for diversity (see Chapter 1), conceptions of what it means for a preservice teacher to be prepared for diversity seem to vary considerably. In his review of the literature on preparing teachers for diversity, Zeichner (1993) identified five major strategies that, in his view, "capture the essence of 'teacher education for diversity' as it is portrayed in the literature” (p. 19). The five approaches he identified were Biography, Cultural Knowledge, Instructional Strategies, Field Experiences, and Attitude Change. This typology provides a useful framework for examining the various approaches to multicultural teacher education. However, based on my own reading of the literature, I choose to add subcategories to two of Zeichner's' categories because I believe that doing so more clearly reflects the different schools of thought on how to go about preparing teachers for diversity. Though I will discuss each strategy separately, 1 don't think that these five strategies are necessarily mutually exclusive. The ways in which they have been written about suggests that there is definitely some overlap between them; in fact, some writers (Banks, 1994; Zeichner, 1993) have advocated the use of some combination of these approaches. Perhaps an ideal program would incorporate elements of all five approaches. io h lf-Awareness The strategy which Zeichner has labeled as Biography, I would prefer to call Self - Awareness because it focuses on the students' acquiring knowledge and understanding of their own cultural identities. Zeichner explains that the goal of this particular strategy is ”helping teacher education students to understand better their own cultural experience and to develop more clarified ethnic and cultural identities“ (p. 15). Proponents of this strategy seem to believe that in order for prospective teachers to work effectively with 40 culturally different students, they first need to develop an awareness and understanding of their own cultural identities. Helping prospective teachers develop awareness of their cultural identities certainly seems to be an excellent starting point for preparing them for diversity, but in my mind, it is only a starting point. I don't believe-nor is it likely that proponents believe-that this awareness alone would prepare teachers to work with diverse learners. This strategy would probably always be used in conjunction with one or more of the other four strategies. Cultural Knowledge According to Zeichner, the focus of this strategy is "to try to overcome the lack of knowledge by teacher education students about the histories of different ethnic groups and their participation in and contributions to life in the United States" (p. 17). My reading of the literature suggests that this category might more appropriately be divided into two subcategories: knowledge of other cultures and knowledge of societal inequities. Knowledge of other cultures. There are some (e. g., Hadaway & Florez, 1987/88; Larke, 1990) who believe that prospective teachers, in order to be prepared for diversity, need to learn about other cultural groups. Thus, these researchers advocate that information about the background and characteristics of various cultural groups be provided to prospective teachers so that they will become more knowledgeable about and sensitive to the needs of these groups. The primary justification for this strategy is the cultural congruence hypothesis discussed earlier in this chapter. Davidman (1990) and Burstein and Cabello (1989), for example, pointed out how research has shown that culturally compatible instruction leads to higher achievement for minority children, while instruction that is culturally insensitive leads to academic failure for these children. While I think it is important for prospective teachers to learn about cultures other than their own, I also have some grave reservations about placing too much emphasis on their acquisition of knowledge about other cultures. The empirical evidence strongly suggests that merely providing prospective teachers with generalizations about various 41 cultural groups is ineffective, if not counterproductive. Not only does this seem to encourage prospective teachers to stereotype students (McDiarmid, 1990), but there is also evidence that they do not utilize the information in their teaching (Grant & Koskela, 1986). A viable conception of preparing teachers for diversity would seem to have to avoid these two dilemmas. Mahan (cited in Grant & Secada, 1990) reported positive attitude outcomes for 291 student teachers who lived and worked in a Native American community for 17 weeks. Noordhoff and Kleinfeld (1993) also reported positive growth for two groups of teacher candidates placed in rural Alaskan communities for a year. The program was designed to replace " generic, decontextualized learning about diverse students” with learning about diverse students in a particular context. Cochran-Smith and Lytle (1992) have also argued that rather than attempting to give prospective teachers "monolithic solutions” to complex problems, they should be taught how to conduct research on their students in a particular context. Thus, effectively preparing teachers for diversity may mean providing them with context-specific rather than generalized information and also teaching them how to learn about their particular students. Knowledge of societal inequities. There are other writers (e. g., see Gillette & Grant, 1991; Trent, 1990) who argue that it is important for prospective teachers to learn about the nature, causes, and consequences of the societal inequities historically and . presently faced by many minority groups in this country. I regard this as a separate subcategory of Cultural Knowledge because the focus seems to be not so much on learning about the cultural groups per se, but more on learning about their historical experiences with racism and discrimination in American society. From this perspective, preparing teachers for diversity means making them aware of societal inequities and why they persist. Ladson-Billings (1991) argues that . . . student teachers need specific factual knowledge of multicultural issues. As teachers, they must be knowledgeable about societal inequity and the competing explanations of its persistence in order to make informed choices among the various pedagogical positions, educational goals, classroom practices, and social visions multicultural education encompasses. (p. 155) 42 Ladson-Billings acknowledges, however, that acquiring such knowledge "does little to predict how students will apply this information in their daily lives and chosen professions” (p. 154). Instructional Strategies For this category, Zeichner (1993) explains that ”prospective teachers are often taught various instructional strategies which are sensitive to cultural and linguistic differences and which enable them to build upon the knowledge and experiences (the cultural resources) that students bring with them to school" (p.19). Once again my reading of the literature suggests that this category might more appropriately be divided into two subcategories because there seem to be at least two distinct ways in which researchers have thought about the need for culturally-sensitive instructional strategies. Some have focused on instructional adaptations to accommodate differences between the culture of the home and the culture of the school, as well as between the culture of the teacher and that of his/her students. Others have focused on adapting instruction to accommodate cultural differences in learning styles. Since both of these schools of thought were discussed in some detail earlier, their treatment here will be rather brief. Cultural drflerences. Some researchers (e. g., Au & Kawakarni, 1994; Burstein & Cabello, 1989; Davidman, 1990; Irvine, 1992; Larke, 1990) have argued that school culture is most compatible with the home culture of majority students, and this compatibility supports their academic performance, whereas for most minority children, incongruities between the home culture and the school culture often result in poor academic performance. Proponents of the cultural difference explanation believe that intercultural knowledge is essential for effective teaching in multicultural learning situations, and they believe that the logical response to the expanding cultural mismatch between teachers and students, as well as between school and home, is to provide multicultural teacher education experiences which inform prospective teachers about various cultures and which encourage them to value cultural differences and not to regard 43 them as deficits. Theoretically, when cultural incongruities have been identified, the teachers will be able to take them into account in designing their instruction. While there seems to be some overlap between this category and the Cultural Knowledge category, the focus here is on leaming about specific features of minority cultures that may be incongruent with features of school culture. In their brief review of literature, Au and Kawakanri (1994) identified five general areas of research on cultural congruence: speakers of dialects, classroom participation structures, narrative and questioning styles, English-as-a-Second-Language students, and the influence of peer groups. Instruction which seeks to address the mismatch between the culture of the home and the culture of the school has been variously referred to as "culturally compatible," "culturally responsive,” ”culturally congruent,” ”culturally appropriate," and "culturally relevant" (see Ladson-Billings, 1994;1995). Distinguishing the differences between these various approaches is beyond the scope of this paper. Learning styles. While learning styles are actually just another type of cultural difference, the quantity of research conducted in this area seems to justify treating them as a separate subcategory of instructional strategies. The basic premise seems to be that children from different cultural groups evince different styles of learning than do majority children and, hence, they require different pedagogy. Proponents of the different learning styles explanation (e.g., Hale, 1982; Pepper & Henry, 1989; Saracho & Gerstl, 1991; Shade, 1989c) believe that poor minority student achievement is due largely to the fact that minority students learn differently, and they further believe that the logical response to the widening cultural disparity between teachers and students is to educate prospective teachers in the learning styles of different cultural groups and in alternative teaching styles that would increase students' likelihood of school success. Fiel riences The field experiences strategy intends to provide opportunities for prospective teachers to have direct contact with children and/or adults from different cultural 44 backgrounds than their own. To proponents of this approach (e. g., Boyle-Baise & Grant, 1991; Noordhoff & Kleinfeld, 1993), preparing teachers for diversity means involving prospective teachers in a diverse learning community and providing multiple, mediated opportunities for them to work and interact with diverse learners. Numerous researchers have remarked about the importance of direct contact with members of other racial/cultural groups. Wayson (cited in Grant & Secada, 1990) assessed the attitudes of a group of student teachers and found that Effective preparation seemed to require, at a minimum, direct contact with students from cultures other than the prospective teacher's combined with translation and interpretation gained from discussion with a knowledgeable and sensitive supervisor, professor, critic teacher or other tutor. . . . (p.412) McGeehan (1982) found that the prospective teachers who demonstrated the most positive attitudes toward a multicultural education course and who Ieamed the most from the course were those who had had ”frequent, high quality previous interethnic experience” (p. 246). Ross and Smith (1992) reported a similar finding. Boyle-Baise and Grant (1991) point out that many teacher candidates have had little or no previous contact with members of different racial/cultural groups and, thus, "they have not had much opportunity to confront and grapple with their knowledge, feelings, and attitudes about people different than themselves” (p. 188). The authors contend that every teacher candidate should have some experience in a diverse setting during his/her preparation program. Some research suggests that it is very important that prospective teachers' field experiences be mediated in order for them to be maximally effective. Bondy, Schmitz, and Johnson (1993), for example, found that teacher candidates who had engaged in volunteer tutoring while enrolled in a multicultural education course all reported positive experiences, whereas, in contrast, ”it appeared that tutoring was a counterproductive experience for tutors who did not take the course" (p. 62). W The goal of developing more positive racial attitudes among prospective teachers is often mentioned in conjunction with one of the preceding strategies, but in my opinion, 45 it doesn't appear to be considered a primary goal by teacher educators-~or maybe the assumption is that positive racial attitudes will be an automatic outcome of students' acquiring more intercultural knowledge. Given the important role that attitudes and beliefs play in shaping what students learn from their education courses (see first two sections of this chapter), this lack of primary attention to promoting attitude change is surprising to me. Most of the proposed strategies for multicultural teacher education seem to take a knowledge-acquisition perspective. Knowledge, however, is not enough because, as Sarap (1991) has pointed out, "people can be well informed and still be racist” (p. 41). Additionally, aside from the unintended consequence of possibly encouraging the development of stereotypes about minority groups, this perspective seems to assume that once prospective teachers have this knowledge, they will be better teachers of minority students. Research by McDiarmid (1992), however, calls this assumption into question. McDiarmid found that when teachers were presented with information about cultural groups, not only did their attitudes not seem to change, but neither did there appear to be any improvement in student achievement (which would be an indicator of improved teaching). Moreland (cited in Pate, 1988) made a critically important point when he wrote, We need to realize that, although sound knowledge is necessary to combat false information, it is not sufficient to change attitude. Facts do not speak for themselves; rather they are interpreted through the experience and biases of those hearing them. (p. 288) A learner's attitudes, as well as what he/she already knows about something, serve as the basis for the interpretation and acquisition of new information (Noordhoff & Kleinfeld, 1993; Shuell, 1986). As McDiarmid and Price (1990) explained, "Prospective teachers, like other learners, reconstruct the information and ideas they encounter to fit into their existing framework" (p. 20). And if the existing attitudes are negative, information on cultural groups may be interpreted negatively (e. g., reinforcing a negative stereotype) or it may even be rejected if it doesn’t fit in with existing beliefs. Some researchers (i.e., Bennett, Niggle, & Stage, 1990; McGeehan, 1982; Ross & Smith, 1992) have found that 46 learners who entered a course with less than positive attitudes toward minority groups tended to learn less from the course than students who entered with more positive attitudes. Maluso (1995) observed that "Some social scientists have suggested that educational approaches addressing prejudice (i.e., those emphasizing attitudes and values) are more effective in reducing racism than those primarily addressing beliefs" (p. 60). The knowledge acquisition approach seems to assume that negative racial attitudes and beliefs can be eradicated by providing the individuals with enough correct information. However, Sarap (1991) explained that the problem with this approach is that . . .it assumes that racism is a false belief system and that it can be dealt with by the application of a superior logic. But, as many social scientists have pointed out, racism does not work in this way, it is not a form of logic but a way of "making sense.” . . . . In other words, reasoned argument based on facts and logic is not enough. This kind of rationalist pedagogy is inadequate because many forms of racism are impervious to reason. (p.41) Hence, it appears that an approach to preparing teachers for diversity that focuses on imparting knowledge and gives scant attention to prospective teachers' entering attitudes and beliefs is unlikely to be very successful. I believe that fostering positive attitude change should be a major focus of any multicultural teacher education effort. James Banks (1994), a leading authority on multicultural education, has also stressed the necessity of making attitude an express focus of attempts to change teachers' attitudes, as he explained, “An extensive review of the research suggests that changing the racial attitudes of adults is a difficult task. To maximize the chances for successful intervention programs, experiences must be designed specifically to change attitudes” (p. 252). To summarize, I have identified five different strategies for multicultural teacher education. Two of these strategies-Cultural Knowledge and Instructional Strategies- place primary emphasis on providing prospective teachers with information about minorities so that they will be able to work more effectively with these students. While the arguments for these approaches are reasonable, they assume that preservice teachers' lack of knowledge is the cause of the problem and that providing information will solve it. Based on the literature that I have reviewed, I would argue that preparing teachers for 47 diversity may be more a matter of developing certain attitudes and propensities than it is acquiring any particular body of knowledge. Conclusions In this chapter I have presented evidence suggesting that just as preservice teachers begin their teacher education programs with set attitudes and beliefs about teaching, so too do they begin with many well-established attitudes toward and beliefs about minority groups. Like their beliefs about teaching, these racial attitudes and beliefs have been Ieamed over a period of many years, and they are not easily susceptible to change. Since we now know that preservice teachers' entering attitudes and beliefs about teaching constrain what they learn about teaching from their education courses, it seems reasonable to assume that their entering attitudes and beliefs about racial groups might also constrain what they learn from courses on diversity. Unfortunately, however, up until now, little research has addressed the impact that prospective teachers' racial attitudes and beliefs might have on their leaming from courses on diversity. The present study is intended to shed more light on how prospective teachers' racial attitudes and belief might influence what they learn from a course on diversity. CHAPTER THREE METHODS In this chapter] will reiterate the purpose of this study and explain how it differs from previous research in this area Then I will describe the research design, the course, the participants, the data sources, and my data analysis procedures. Purpose The purposes of this study were embedded in the three guiding research questions. One purpose was to identify prospective teachers' attitudes and beliefs relative to racial/ethnic minority groups and then to determine whether the preservice teachers who expressed favorable attitudes toward racial minority groups differed substantially from those who expressed unfavorable attitudes. Another purpose was to determine how and to what extent students' racial attitudes and beliefs changed during the course of the semester. A final purpose was to determine whether there appeared to be important differences between students with initially favorable racial attitudes and those with . initially unfavorable ones in terms of their perceptions of the course or their learning from the course. This study differs from comparable eariier research on the impact of multicultural teacher education courses in four major ways. First, unlike comparable earlier studies (Bennett, Niggle, & Stage, 1990; Bondy, Schmitz, & Johnson 1993; Larke, 1990; McGeehan, 1982; Reed, 1993; Ross & Smith, 1992; Tran, Young, & Di Lella, 1994) where attitude change generally seemed to be regarded as an outcome of knowledge growth, this study takes the perspective that knowledge growth may be instead, to a great extent, an outcome of the students' attitudes. In all but one (Ross & Smith) of these 49 studies, the researchers administered a pretest of students' attitudes and/or beliefs at the beginning of a multicultural course and then a posttest afterwards. They then discussed whether or not the multicultural course appeared to have had any impact on the students' attitudes and/or beliefs. The implicit assumption in each case seemed to be that knowledge gained from the course may be expected to affect the students' attitudes and/or beliefs. Among this group of researchers, only McGeehan (1982) suggested that there might be a relationship between the students' entering attitudes and beliefs and their knowledge growth from the course. This is the perspective taken in the present study. Second, this study utilized more than one measure of student attitudes. Most of the studies cited earlier in this paragraph relied on a single measure of students' attitudes and beliefs; the exceptions were McGeehan (1982), Reed (1993), and Ross and Smith (1992). In this dissertation study, students' attitudes and beliefs were assessed through two different instruments, as well as through multiple interviews. The use of the interviews provided a richer data set and a means of corroborating students' responses on the pencil-and-paper instruments. Third, this study provides statistical data suggesting that the students who were interviewed were, in fact, representative of the students in the class as a whole. The researchers who employed interviews to assess students' attitudes and beliefs relied primarily (McGeehan, 1982) or exclusively (Ross & Smith, 1992) on volunteer participants, and they had to assume that these volunteers were representative of students in the course as a whole. Fourth, the target students in the present study were interviewed more frequently than were the students in the studies by McGeehan (1982), where some students were interviewed three times, and Ross and Smith (1992), where students were interviewed twice. In contrast, the target students in this study were interviewed a total of six times each over the course of the semester, thus providing me with a more detailed picture of each students' experience of the class. 50 This study also builds on and extends previous research (McGeehan, 1982; Ross & Smith, 1992) which has suggested that the students who seem to gain the most from multicultural education experiences are those who enter the course with the most accepting attitudes toward minorities. This study contributes to the field by investigating whether and how students with more accepting racial attitudes gain more from multicultural courses than students with less accepting attitudes. Research Design and Rationale This study combines elements of quantitative and qualitative research methodology. My decision to employ both quantitative and qualitative techniques was rooted in my belief that the combination of techniques would enable me to paint a more complete picture of the impact that the CEP 240 course (Diverse Learners in Multicultural Perspective) seemed to have on different students than I would have been able to do with either methodology by itself. The quantitative data gave me broad information about the students as a whole and about the course's impact on them. Then, by conducting statistical analyses of the quantitative data, I could determine whether or not observed differences between groups were statistically significant. In addition, with the results of these statistical analyses I have more basis for generalizing beyond my particular sample than I would have if I were relying on qualitative data alone. On the other hand, the qualitative data gave me more detailed information on how a smaller, volunteer sample of students experienced the course. It also provided additional background on the students and highlighted other ways in which those with more favorable racial attitudes were similar to and different from those with less favorable attitudes. Finally, the qualitative data gave me a close-up look at how several individuals responded to specific components of the course and the meanings that they seemed to attach to their experiences in the course. 51 Course I chose to study the CEP 240 course because it explicitly and exclusively focuses on diversity issues and also because it is a course taken by many prospective teachers. As a prerequisite for admission into the teacher education program, prospective teachers must enroll in either CEP 240 or TB 250 (Human Diversity, Power and Opportunity in Social Institutions). Consequently, most students who enroll in this course do so because it is required for the teacher education program. During the semester of this study, for example, 201 students were enrolled, and out of the 196 students who completed a questionnaire, 174 (or 89%) indicated that they planned to go into some area of education. Typically, about three quarters of the students enrolled in CEP 240 are females, and about the same percentage are freshmen or sophomores. Additionally, over 90% of the students usually fall within the 18-23 age group. While all racial groups were represented in the course, generally about 80-90% of the students are White Americans and 6-10% are African Americans. Students from other racial backgrounds, including some international students, typically comprise another 48% of the course enrollment. While this research study focused only on the racial attitudes and beliefs of prospective teachers, it should be noted that the course itself had a much broader focus. The course is intended to explore a wide range of diversity, including racial/ethnic, cultural, linguistic, gender, and cognitive differences. According to the course syllabus, two primary objectives of the course are to "examine the nature of diversity itself‘ and to ”foster sensitivity and respect for diverse learners." The course content is organized into three main strands. One strand looks at the nature of diverse Iearners--who they are and what their major characteristics are. A second strand examines the nature and impact of various sociocultural contexts. The third strand focuses on the implications of diversity for schooling and on practical school and classroom applications. A detailed outline of the course is included in Appendix A along, with the reading list and sample activities. 52 Students enrolled in the course met together for two hours for lecture each Thursday for fifteen weeks. They also met in groups of approximately 25 students for two hours each Tuesday in eight different lab (discussion) sections. During the semester of the study, the course was team-taught by two African American male professors, who conducted the Thursday lecture sessions. The eight lab sections were taught by five different graduate assistants: three White females, an African female, and myself (an African American male). After each Thursday lecture session, the two professors met with the five lab instructors to discuss the activities to be included in the lab session for the following Tuesday. Consequently, there was considerable consistency in content and activities across all of the lab sessions, although there was often some variation in the particular techniques the lab instructors used to cover the planned content or to do the activities. The weekly meetings of instructors were also used regularly for problem- solving, idea generation, and on-going evaluation of the course. During the weekly lecture sessions, the two professors typically reiterated and/or extended some of the major points from the assigned readings, but they also presented new material that was not in the readings. The professors regularly attempted to actively involve students in the lecture sessions through various brief activities (e.g., having them do short writing exercises, asking some students to share relevant experiences, having particular students seek out certain information and then report back to the whole class, showing and discussing brief videotape segments) and by encouraging them to ask questions during the lectures. In addition, the professors occasionally engaged in roleplaying, where they would argue the opposing sides of the issue currently under discussion. Approximately half of the lecture sessions featured guest speakers, including a blind student, a bilingual Latina professor, a physically disabled professor, two deaf graduate students, a panel of representatives from local community service organizations, and a panel of representatives from the campus gay/lesbian organization. 53 The weekly lab sessions promoted student involvement with the course material through a variety of activities. Students viewed and discussed several videotapes, including a video on racial discrimination, one on blindness, one on learning disabilities, and two on inclusion. They also discussed several readings from the course packet (see Appendix A). In addition, several times during the semester students participated in roleplaying activities which focused on such issues as racial discrimination, school desegregation, immigration, abortion, gay/lesbian rights, and sex discrimination. Students also participated in a number of cooperative learning activities, including reading and collectively responding to several short scenarios describing problem pupils or classroom situations. Other activities included a blind walk to enable students to experience blindness and a series of exercises to help students experience having a learning disability. Finally, the lab sessions provided students with opportunities to discuss their reactions to or raise questions about the lecture presentations by the course professors or by guest speakers. Of the five multicultural teacher education strategies described in the previous chapter, only the field experience strategy was not included as part of this course. The biography strategy was in evidence in perhaps two sessions early in the semester when students were asked to identify their own cultural and ethnic backgrounds. The instructional strategies approach was very much in evidence in the course, as a lot of information about cultural differences and about different learning styles was presented. One course session focused on the use of cooperative learning and another presented strategies for working with learning disabled students, but beyond those two sessions, specific instructional strategies were rarely presented. On the other hand, students were presented with a great deal of information about a wide variety of differences between people, not just cultural differences. Students were taught about the various ways in which mismatches between the culture of the home and the culture of the school might occur. Learning style differences were also presented and discussed, but not nearly to the 54 extent that cultural differences were. In addition to highlighting differences between various racial/ethnic and cultural groups, a considerable portion of the course was devoted to study of special needs individuals, including the deaf, the blind, the mentally retarded, and the learning disabled. Another strategy that was used frequently in the course was the cultural knowledge strategy. While there was some attention given to the subcategory of knowledge about cultural groups (especially deaf culture and gay/lesbian culture), I would say that the great majority of what students Ieamed about various cultural groups was directly related to the education of these groups. In other words, most of the information presented dealt with various factors (language, child-rearing practices, learning styles, cultural norms, etc.) that need to be considered in teaching culturally different students. The subcategory of knowledge of societal inequities received considerably more attention. With nearly every minority group discussed in the course, there was some discussion of how the group has historically been (and/or presently is being) discriminated against. For example, students were exposed to a number of landmark legislation and legal decisions that have affected the education of minority groups (including individuals with special needs). Usually included was some explanation of the background for and the impact (both positive and negative) of the legislation or court decision for the targeted groups. Finally, the attitude change strategy was also in evidence to a limited extent. While attitude change was one of the goals of the course, in my opinion, as one of the instructors of the course, students' attitudes toward and beliefs about racial minorities or about the other types of diversity focused on in the course received little direct attention. There was some discussion of race as a system for classifying people and also some discussion of what constitutes a minority group. Students were exposed to a great deal of information about different types of minority groups, and they were also exposed to representatives of various minorities who appeared as guest speakers in the lecture sessions at different points during the course. Apparently, the intention was that this extensive exposure would lead to the formation of 55 more favorable racial attitudes and beliefs. In my perception, there were few occasions in the course when students racial attitudes and beliefs were dealt with directly, when they were made explicit and then confronted and challenged, as McDiarmid and Price (1990) suggested that they need to be. One activity that did seem to directly address student attitudes was the role-playing that occurred during several of the lab sessions. Some research has shown (Maluso, 1995) that role-playing can be an effective tool for changing people's attitudes; however, role-playing was only a small portion of CEP 240. Participants Virtually all of the 201 students enrolled in CEP 240 were participants in this study at some level. All students completed a pre- and post-questionnaire, and the attendance, exams, and course grades were analyzed for all students. Additionally, during the second week of the course, 31 students volunteered to complete an attitude survey and to participate in a pre- and post-interview. Students enrolled in my lab section were not allowed to volunteer for this portion of the research. Frnally, on the basis of their age and their responses to the attitude survey, I selected 14 target students from the 31 volunteers and interviewed them at 2-3 week intervals throughout the semester. I chose to limit this group to students in the 18—22 age range because this is the most typical age range for prospective teachers. The three levels of student participation in this study and the composition of each group are illustrated in Table 3-1. Table 3.1 Participants in the Study Teacher Students 11 Gender Race Age Group Candidates 149 Females = 76% 167 White = 85% 18-20 = 76% 174 = 89% All 196 47 Males = 24% 16 Black = 8% 21-23 = 17% 13 other = 7% 24-28 = 6% 29-50 = 1% 26 Females = 84% 28 White = 90% 18-20 = 65% 30 = 97% Voltnlru 31 5 Males = 16% 3 Black = 10% 21-23 = 13% 24—28 = 16% 29-50 = 6% Target 14 11 Females = 79% 14 White = 100% 1&20 = 8‘6_% 13 = 93% 3 Males = 21% 21-23 = 14% 56 Data Sources In addition to using course records of attendance, exam scores, and final grades as data sources, I relied on four different instruments to gather data about students: a teaching/learning questionnaire, a racial attitude survey, a two-part structured interview, and a series of unstructured interviews. Table 3.2 outlines the data sources that were available for the various participants in the study. Table 3.2 Data Sources for Participants in Study Students Data Sources All Quick Discrimination Index (posttest only) Teaching/Learning Questionnaire (pre and post) Course records Quick Discrimination Index (pre and post) Volunteer Teaching/Learning Questionnaire (pre and post) Course records Pre and post structured interviews Quick Discrimination Index (pre and post) Target Teaching/Learning Questionnaire (pre and post) Course records Pre and post structured interviews Four unstructured interviews (during semester) Teachin 'n uestionnaire This instrument (see Appendix B) was one used by researchers in a study of the Accepting Behaviors for Cultural Diversity for Teachers (ABCD) Project (McDiarmid & Price, 1990); however, most of the items were drawn from a questionnaire originally developed by the National Center for Research on Teacher Education (N CRTE), which is located at Michigan State University. McDiarmid and Price reported no reliability information for the ABCD questionnaire, and there was also none reported on the original NCRTE questionnaire. The purpose of the questionnaire-and the reason it was selected for use in this study-«was to assess students' beliefs about teaching and learning, as well as their awareness of and attitudes toward issues of diversity. The questionnaire was administered to all CEP 240 students in their lab sections during the first week of the 57 course and then readministered in the same manner at the end of the course. Though not graded, the questionnaire was considered by the instructors as part of the course work, and the students were asked to identify themselves on them. Attitude Survey The attitude survey (see Appendix C) was a combination and adaptation of Ponterotto and Burkard's Quick Discrimination Index (Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993) and the Bogardus Social Distance Scale (Bogardus, 1925). I chose these two instruments because they were short and there was evidence that each was a reliable measure of people's attitudes (Law & Lane, 1987; Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993). I reduced the Quick Discrimination Index (QDI) from 25 items to 10 items by deleting all items relating to gender issues and retaining only those items relating to racial issues. I then wrote five original statements to bring the total to 15 items. The QDI items were answered using a 7-point Likert scale with responses ranging from Strongly Agree to Strongly Disagree. To avoid positively worded response bias, seven of the QDI items were worded negatively. Using the split-half technique and the Spearrnan Brown prophecy formula, I obtained a reliability of .80 for the QDI. A reliability coefficient of .80 can be considered as useful, but coefficients of .85 and higher are preferable (McDaniel, 1994). I modified the Bogardus Social Distance Scale by limiting it to only five minority groups (Whites, Blacks, Hispanics, Asian Americans, and Native Americans) rather than the 36-39 groups originally included. In addition, I rephrased several of the items. The Social Distance Scale was also scored with a 7-point Likert scale, but on this instrument there was no way to compensate for positively worded response bias. I administered both parts of this attitude survey at the beginning and at the end of the course to the 31 students who volunteered to be interviewed. This survey was not part of the students' course work and was administered to them individually or in small groups outside of regular class time. I asked the students to identify themselves on the survey so that I 58 would able to compare their individual posttest responses with their pretest responses. I did not compute a reliability coefficient for the Social Distance Scale because I elected to not analyze this data because of an obvious lack of variability in students' responses to the items. I found that, with the notable exception of the item referring to maintaining an intimate relationship, nearly every student marked a seven (the highest possible response) for every item. This circumstance suggested to me that students may have been giving what they considered to be socially desirable responses. Administering the attitude survey to the 31 volunteers had three purposes. First, the survey provided more specific information about student attitudes toward minority groups than the general questionnaire administered to all students. Second, students' responses to the survey assisted me in selecting the 12 target students by giving me another measure of the students' level of sensitivity to diversity issues. Finally, I had additional data on the 17 students not selected for the target group, providing me with a more complete picture of their growth over the semester. During the first two weeks of the course and again at the end of the course, I administered the QDI and the Social Distance Scale to the 31 volunteer students. Because my interviews with the volunteer students convinced me that the QDI had done a good job of distinguishing between individuals with more favorable racial attitudes and those with less favorable attitudes, I made the decision to administer the QDI to all students at the end of the course as an attachment to the ABCD questionnaire. S I 'ew I administered two structured interviews (see Appendix D) to the 31 volunteers, one near the beginning and one near the end of the course. The first structured interview was designed to obtain additional information about students' backgrounds, their prior experiences with minority groups, their expectations for the course, and their beliefs and feelings about teaching certain racial groups. The second structured interview had a threefold purpose: 1) obtaining students' evaluations of the course and of their own 59 learning, 2) reassessing students’ attitudes toward selected racial groups, and 3) eliciting their beliefs and feelings about teaching certain racial groups. There were several reasons why I chose to use structured interviews as one of my data sources. First, they would provide a systematic means of securing the same background information about each of the participants. Second, the structured interviews would enable me to compare students' responses in the second interview with their responses to the same questions in the first interview, and through this comparison I could identify possible changes in students' beliefs and/or attitudes. Furthermore, having all students respond to the same questions would enable to me to compare responses across students as well as across interviews. Most of the structured interviews were conducted during the first two weeks and the last two weeks of the course. The interviews typically lasted slightly more than one hour each. So that I could tape record each interview, I always met with the participants in a quiet place, and in every case I met with each of them individually and in private-no other people were ever present during the structured interviews. The majority of the interviews took place in a small office in the education building, but for the convenience of some of the participants, I conducted some interviews in their dormitories or, in one case, in a student's apartment. Unstructured Interviews I conducted a series of unstructured interviews with the 14 target students at two— to three-week intervals throughout the semester. I had four unstructured interviews with 13 of the 14 target students and three with the remaining student. The purpose of the unstructured interviews was to assess what students were learning in the course and what their attitudes/feelings were toward what they were learning. Typically, there were two questions which I asked in each interview: What stands out for you about the last two weeks? During the last two weeks, have you learned anything that you consider to be important? The remainder of the questions for these interviews were not predetermined; 60 rather, I just asked students to talk about how they felt about what was happening in the course and about what they were learning. Typical questions during the unstructured interviews were like the following: What do you think was the purpose of [a particular activity]? How did you feel about this activity? The rationale for including this instrument was that it would enable me to more effectively track the students' perceptions of the course, their learning, and their attitudes toward various diversity issues. Having several different instruments getting at the same information in different ways was a way of triangulating and making sure that I had a rich data set for each target student on each construct of interest. The unstructured interviews varied in length from about 40 minutes to over an hour. As with the structured interviews, they were all conducted in a quiet, private place so that they could be tape recorded. The great majority of these interviews were conducted in a small office in the education building, and the rest were conducted in the students' dormitories. Data Analysis As noted earlier, I collected both quantitative and qualitative data for this study. First, I will explain my procedures for analyzing the quantitative data, then for the qualitative data. Quantitative Data There were three types of quantitative data that I analyzed in this study: attitude surveys, teaching/learning questionnaires, and course records. Attitude Surveys Students' responses to each item on the QDI were scored on a seven-point scale, with the low score being 1 for a response that indicated an unfavorable attitude toward racial minorities and the high score being 7 for a response that indicated a favorable attitude. The sum of the individual scores for the 15 items on the survey yielded a total score which I interpreted as an indication of how positively or negatively each student felt toward racial minorities. Once all the surveys had been scored, I divided the students into 61 three groups based on the total score that they received: high scorers, middle scorers, and low scorers. Next, I conducted a chi-square analysis to determine whether or not there were significant differences between high scorers and low scorers on any of the 15 items on the QDI. Finally, for the 31 volunteer students, who were the only ones to take the QDI both at the beginning and at the end of the course, I conducted an ANOVA to determine whether there was a difference between high, middle, and low scorers in terms of the change in their scores from the pretest to the posttest. Teachin in uestionnaires There were a total of 25 items on the ABCD questionnaire, with the first 14 items being scored on a 7-point Likert scale. For each of these 14 items I conducted a chi - square analysis to determine whether there were significant differences in the responses of students who had scored high on the QDI and those who had scored low. In addition, because the ABCD questionnaire was administered pre and post to all students, I conducted an ANOVA on each item to determine whether there were differences between high scorers and low scorers in terms of the change in their scores from the pretest to the posttest. The remaining items on the ABCD questionnaire were scored somewhat differently. The next six items, #15 through #20, each offered respondents a choice of six answers; however, since none of the answers could truly be considered a "best" answer, a rating scale could not be used. Also, because no choice was considered better than another, there was no basis for determining whether changes from the pretest to the posttest were positive, neutral, or negative. For these reasons, the results of this section of the questionnaire were difficult to interpret. I concluded that the most logical way to report the data from this section was to list the choices that were most frequently selected and indicate the percentages of highs and lows selecting each of these choices. However, these six items contributed nothing to my understanding of the students in this study. Items #21 through 23 each included between four and seven answer choices. For 62 these items I devised a four-point scoring scheme in which I awarded 1 point for the answer which indicated the least accepting attitude and 4 points for the answer which indicated the most accepting attitude. For item #24, students had a choice of four responses, with no one response being the "best” response. Finally, item #25 called for a short answer from the respondents. I again used the four-point scoring scheme in which I awarded one point for a response indicating strong disagreement with the item and four points for a response indicating strong agreement. The scoring for items #21 through 23 and for #25 was subjective, but one of the CEP 240 course instructors and I, after some discussion, were in 100% agreement on which responses constituted the "best” responses for these items. As I did for the first fourteen items on the instrument, I conducted a chi - square analysis of items #15 through #25 to determine whether there were any significant differences in the responses of students who had scored high on the QDI and those who had scored low. In addition, I also conducted an ANOVA of the pre and post scores to determine whether there were differences from the pretest to the posttest. Course Records The course records that I analyzed at the end of the semester included attendance records, midterm and final examination grades, and final course grades. Attendance. I collected the attendance records that had been maintained by each instructor of the lab sections. I converted the attendance for each student in the class into a percentage by dividing the number of class sessions for which the student was present by the total number of class sessions. Thus, the highest possible score for attendance would be 100%, a score achieved by students who had attended every class session. After individual attendance scores had been determined, I then used an analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test whether or not there were significant differences between the three groups being studied: those who had scored high on the QDI, those who had scored in the middle range, and those who had scored low. 63 Midterm and final exams. I analyzed students' midterm and final examinations in much the same way as I did their attendance records. The highest possible score on each examination was 100% (though, actually, a few students scored a couple of percentage points higher because of the extra credit questions). Once I had obtained from the other instructors the examination scores for each student, I again used an ANOVA to determine whether or not there were differences in the performance of students who had scored high, middle, and low on the QDI. Course grades. I obtained each student's final course grade through examining a computer printout of student grades a couple of weeks after the conclusion of the course. Students were graded on the following four-point scale: 1.0 = D, 2.0 = C, 3.0 = B, and 4.0 = A. Once again I used an ANOVA to test for differences between the three groups of students Qualitative Data There were two types of qualitative data analyzed in this study: structured interviews and unstructured interviews. Structured Interviews The first step in analyzing the structured interviews was obtaining verbatim transcripts of each interview with the fourteen target students. I personally transcribed almost half of the structured interviews, and the remaining half were transcribed professionally. To ensure the complete accuracy of each interview that was professionally transcribed, I listened to the tape of the entire interview and edited the transcripts as necessary. Each speaker's comments in each interview were numbered for easy reference. The next step in the process was to begin to organize the data to facilitate its analysis. I made a chart which outlined the background information provided by each student about his/her family, high school, and hometown. I then pulled together in one document each student's response to each of the remaining interview questions. Putting 64 all students' responses to a question together in one document facilitated the process of looking for themes in the data and for similarities and differences across students. While in the process of organizing my data, I concurrently identified eight of the target students--four who scored high on the QDI and four who scored low--that seemed to me to be the most interesting cases, and I beg’n to analyze their interviews. In deciding on these eight students, I considered the extent to which their comments in the interviews seemed to corroborate their QDI scores and provide additional bases for understanding their racial attitudes and beliefs. I also considered how articulate the students were in expressing their ideas, because some were much better than others at explaining themselves cleariy and in more detail. For these eight students I began to construct another set of charts in which I listed from each of their two structured interviews all of their remarks that appeared relevant to the guiding research questions of the study. In one column of the chart I wrote my own brief commentaries on their remarks, noting important ideas, contradictions, and possible themes, and also raising questions for future thought and analysis. As the charts for each student were completed, I wrote a brief summary of his/her two interviews, along with a commentary stating whether or not there appeared to have been any changes from the pre-interview to the post-interview and whether there appeared to be any major themes or patterns in the student's remarks. In addition, I discussed these summaries and commentaries with the chair of my dissertation committee to check out the conclusions I was reaching and to further prod my thinking about the data. It was this process of analysis, discussion, and reflection that led to me select three participants--two lows and one highrfo-as students to focus on for more detailed analysis. As the final step in my analysis of the structured interviews, I analyzed the responses for each question separately, looking for possible patterns or themes across the fourteen interviews. In my analysis a pattern or theme became a category when I developed a working definition to distinguish which responses could be included. This 65 , final step in the analysis process involved reading and re-reading the interview responses because as new categories emerged from my analysis, I would revisit previously analyzed interviews to seek additional evidence to confirm or disconfirrn the new category. Sometimes, in so doing, my definitions of the categories had to be revised. Because these were structured interviews, obviously most of the categories were shaped by the questions that were asked. The questions included in the structured interview were designed to get at the particular information that, based on my reading of the literature and on the specific purposes of this study, I felt I would need to properly address my research questions. Once a category had been identified, I looked for similar responses from at least four of the seven high students or four of the seven low students. If I found less than four similar responses among students in the same group, the category was dropped from my analysis. If I did find four similar responses among students at one level (i.e., high or low), I then looked for similar or different responses from at least three of the students at the other level. If I did not find at least three corresponding or contrasting responses among the other group of students, this category was also dropped. Thus, the only categories included in my written report were those for which I had clear supporting evidence (either corresponding or contrasting) for at least half of the students. The ten categories that were included fell into four broad areas and are outlined below. Background Experiences 1. Community characteristics 2. Friendships with racial minorities 3. Awareness of racism and discrimination Beliefs about the Education of Minorities l. Minorities' opportunities to learn 2. Explanation of lower minority achievement 3. Teaching minority students Impact of the Course 1. Most important leamings 2. Feelings about the course Reactions to Being Interviewed 1. Effect of being interviewed 2. Response to a Black interviewer Although some additional categories of responses did emerge during my analysis, they were all eventually dropped due to lack of sufficient supporting evidence. For example, there was some evidence that students who scored high on the QDI f elt more empathy for others than did students who scored low, but based on my criteria for retaining a category, this idea did not appear in enough of the students' interviews. Similarly, there was some evidence that students who scored low on the QDI were more likely to favor maintaining the status quo in society and in schools than were students who scored high, but again I did not find enough instances of supporting evidence to include this category in my final analysis. Unstructured Interviews I analyzed the unstructured interviews of only three target students--two students who scored low on the QDI and one student who scored high. I chose to analyze the interviews of these three students because, based on my impressions from interviewing them and on my close analysis of their pre and post structured interviews, they seemed to represent the three different responses that students seemed to have to the course. Claire represented students who seemed mostly unaffected by the course, Joy represented those who were stimulated to seriously reexamine their existing racial attitudes and beliefs, and Terri represented those for whom the course primarily served as an affirmation of their already favorable racial attitudes and beliefs. For each of these three students, I began by listening to the four unstructured interviews that I had conducted at 2-3 week intervals during the semester of the study. I 67 transcribed verbatim everything in each interview that seemed related to their racial attitudes and beliefs, and in a separate document] summarized everything else. After the transcriptions of the students' comments about racial issues were finished, I analyzed them for patterns and themes in much the same fashion as I had done for the structured interviews. However, it should be noted that because these three students were selected to be illustrative of three different responses to the course, I was most interested in highlighting how they differed. Therefore, since this analysis focused on only three students, I included a category only if I found contrasting responses from at least two of the three students. In formulating my categories for these students, I also referred back to their responses in the two structured interviews. I was able to identify four major categories of difference between the three students: 1) background experiences, including their previous interracial experiences, their attitudes and beliefs about racial minorities, and their levels of awareness of racism and discrimination, 2) reactions to the content of the course, 3) leamings gained from the course, and 4) personal characteristics, specifically, their openness to new information and their ability to be self -refiective. CHAPTER FOUR RESULTS AND DISCUSSION In this chapter I present the results from my analyses of the quantitative data, which will provide a broad view of the volunteer students and CEP 240 students as a whole, and from my analyses of the qualitative data, which will provide a more close-up view of the target students and their experience of the course. Because my primary interest is how to better prepare a mostly White teaching force to work with an increasingly diverse student population, I elected to consider only White students in all of my analyses of the data. In the quantitative section, I will begin by discussing students' responses to the racial attitude survey which was administered to all students. Second, I will examine some of the other quantitative data collected from the 28 volunteer students who were White, as well as from the White nonvolunteers in the class. Next, in the qualitative section, I will discuss my analysis of selected qualitative data on the 14 target students. Finally, I will discuss the qualitative data on three illustrative target students. Quantitative Data The three types of quantitative data that I collected for all students were 1) the Quick Discrimination Index (QDI), which assessed students' racial attitudes, 2) the ABCD questionnaire, which assessed students' views on teaching and learning, and 3) course records, which included attendance, exam grades, and course grades. Although the QDI was administered only to the 28 volunteer students at the beginning of the course, it was administered to all students at the end of the course. The ABCD questionnaire was administered pre and post to all students. Records of students' 69 attendance, exam grades, and course grades were not examined until after the conclusion of the course. The Quick Discrimination Index As I indicated above, the QDI was administered to all CEP 240 students at the conclusion of the course. Only the 28 volunteer students completed the survey both at the beginning and at the end of the course. In my original research design, I had not planned to use the QDI with the entire class, only with the volunteers; however, late in the semester] realized that administering the QDI to the whole class would facilitate my making comparisons between the attitudes of volunteers and nonvolunteers. Therefore, I made the decision to have all students complete the QD] at the end of the semester. First, I will discuss the results of the end-of-semester, whole-class administration; then I will discuss the results of the pre and post administration to the volunteer students. All Students ' The lowest possible score on the QDI was 15, indicating highly unfavorable attitudes toward racial minorities, and the highest possible score was 105, indicating highly favorable attitudes. Students' actual scores ranged from 48 to 101, with the mean score being 75.01. As Figure 4.1 illustrates, the distribution of scores on the QDI was bimodal but still approximated a normal distribution. For the purposes of my analysis, I defined high scorers as students who scored one-half standard deviation or more above the mean of the class, and I defined low scorers as students who scored one-half standard deviation or more below the mean. In other words, students who scored 81 or higher on the QDI were considered to be high scorers, and students who scored 69 or lower were considered to be low scorers. My rationale for selecting these particular cutoff points was that, theoretically, approximately 34% of any given population will score between one-half standard deviation above the mean and one-half standard deviation below the mean, with approximately 33% of the remaining population scoring higher and 33% scoring lower. Thus, when I compare the high scorers in my sample to the low scorers, 70 Figure 4.1 Distribution of scores on Quick Discrimination Index (QDI) 3O 27 :4 /‘\ /‘\ .8 / \7 \ Mueller 3 \ < / 6 3/1 : rrfirrrriirrr seaseassaaa§§ l-O semasezeaasgg 0-! Quick Discrimination Index (QDI) Seas I would be comparing the top third of the sample to the bottom third. However, because the distribution of scores in my sample was not perfectly normal, there was some variation in the size of the high, middle, and low groups. There were 49 students (33%) who were high scorers, 55 students (37%) who were low scorers, and 45 students (30%) who scored in the middle range. Analysis of scores on the QDI revealed that for each of the fifteen items there was a significant difference between the responses of students who scored high (more favorable attitudes) and those of students who scored low (less favorable attitudes). In fact, as Table 4.1 illustrates, for twelve of the fifteen items, the differences between the two groups were highly significant (p < .0000). Although we know that high scorers and low scorers, by definition, have responded differently to the items on the survey, it is from these different responses that we may be able to infer differences in racial attitudes. Therefore, taking a closer look at the pattern of differences in their responses is the best 71 way to understand in what particular ways the members of the two groups appeared to differ in their racial attitudes. Rather than discussing each of the fifteen items separately, I have grouped related items into seven broad categories: perceptions of minorities, recognition of racism/discrimination, views on multicultural education, friendships with minorities, views on interracial relationships, views on living/working with minorities, and sensitivity to minority issues. Perceptions of minorities. Items number 5, 11, and 13 provided information about how students viewed minorities. Ninety-two percent of the high scorers (highs) strongly or mostly disagreed with the idea that ”Some racial minorities are more of a liability than an asset to our country," but less than half of the low scorers (lows) disagreed. Similarly, 78% of the highs mostly or strongly disagreed with the idea that minorities aren't willing to work hard, but only 26% of the lows disagreed. Finally, 84% of the highs regarded themselves as unprejudiced toward any minority group, whereas only 36% of the lows considered themselves as unprejudiced. Students' responses to these three items indicate that one difference between the two groups is that that low scorers' perceptions of minorities are significantly less favorable than are high scorers' perceptions. Specifically, lows appear to have less appreciation for racial minorities, are three times as likely as highs to regard minorities as unwilling to work hard, and are far less likely to consider themselves as unprejudiced toward minorities. Recognition of racism/discrimination. Students' responses to items number 7 and 12 served to indicate their awareness of the scope of the problem of racism and discrimination. None of the high scorers believed that minorities complain too much about racial discrimination and 67% strongly or mostly disagreed with that statement; in contrast, 25% of the lows mostly or strongly agreed that minorities do complain too much, and only 13% disagreed. On the question of whether or not White racism is still a major problem in this country, 75% of the highs believed that it is, as compared to only 31% of the lows. The differences in students' responses to these two items suggest that ~I‘LI 2 A Table 4.1 QDI Item 72 Student Responses to QDI Items % Strongly or Mostly Agreeing % Strongly (r Mostly Disagreeing “has Lows Chi- square Value P Wrceptions of Minorities S. Someracial minorities aremoreofa liability than an asset to our country. 11. More minorities would be successful if they were willing to work hard. 13. I am not prejudiced toward any minority groups. 1.8% 91.9% 47.3% 37.669 13h: Lows 0% 0% 7.3% 77.6% 25.5% 36.167 83 .7% 36.3% 0% 7.3% 28.548 Recomition of RacimDiscrlm. 7. Overall,] think minorities complain too much about racial discrimination. 12. White people's racism toward racial minority groups still constitutes a ma'or blem in America 0% 25.4% 67.4% 12.8% 39.333 75.5% 30.9% 4.1% 12.7% 30.724 Multicultural Education 9. Schools should promote traditional American values as well as values of the culturally diverse students in the class. 10. Schools should require all students to learn about the contributions of 71.4% 52.8% § 5.4% 13.651 .0337 85.7% 60.0% § 1.8% 16.81] 1. My friendship network is very racially mixed. 6. My closest friends are from my own racial group. various minori ups to our society. Ffiendshifi 57.2% 16.3% 6.1% 30.9% 29.766 53% 88.3% 22.4% 29.110 ‘ Interracial Reiationshig 3. I would feel O.K. about my son or daughter dating someone from a different race. 8. Itisbetterifpeoplemarrywithintheir own race. 87.8% 18.2% 14.5% 56.497 18.2% 69.4% 18.2% 42.096 ‘ E::Zfiorkini with Minorities 14. I would enjoy living in a neighborhood with a racially diverse pqiulation (i.e., Whites, Blacks, Hispanics, Asians, etc). 15. I would enjoy teaching in a school with a racially inverse populatim. 91.9% 34.5% 12.7% 41.551 l(X)% 47.2% 3.6% 55.580 nsitivi to Minorit Issu 2. Affirmative action programs in business and education constitute reverse discrimination. 4. Itbothers me thataradai minority person has never been President of the United States. 10.2% 32.7% 22.4% 3.6% 15.150 .0191 63.3% 7.2% 2% 23.7% 42.717 73 high scorers appear to be much more aware of and better informed about racism and discrimination than are low scorers, and/or perhaps they are just more compassionate and more sensitive to minority concerns than are low scorers. Views on multicultural education. Two items on the QDI, #9 and #10, addressed the issue of multicultural education. Although lows and highs were in closer agreement on these two items than on any others on the survey, their responses were still significantly different. About 71% of the high scorers, compared to 53% of the low scorers, believed that schools should promote the values of those who are culturally different. Also, 85% of the highs, compared to 60% of the lows, believed that all students should learn about the contributions of minority groups. Students' responses to these two items indicate that highs and lows appear to differ in their commitment to multicultural education. In other words, the responses of the high scorers suggest that they are more likely than lows to be supportive of education that recognizes the diversity in our society. Friendships with minorities. Items number 1 and 6 asked students about their friendships with individuals from different racial groups. Neariy 60% of the high scorers agreed that their friendship networks were racially mixed, as compared to only 16% of the low scorers. Although over 50% of the highs reported that their closest friends were White, nearly 90% of the lows reported the same thing. Furthermore, over 22% of the highs indicated that they had close friends from a different racial background, whereas none of the lows made such an indication. Thus, another apparent difference between the two groups is that highs appear to be considerably more likely to report having friendships-sometimes close friendships--with racial minorities than are lows. It seems particulariy noteworthy that none of the lows reported having close friends from a different racial group. Nevertheless, from these data it is impossible to determine whether this lack of close friendships is in any way related to the low scorers' racial attitudes. 74 Interracial relationships. Items number 3 and 8 were designed to elicit students' attitudes toward interracial relationships. While almost 90% of the highs indicated that they would have no problem with their son or daughter dating someone from a different race, only about 20% of the lows made the same indication. Additionally, nearly 15% of the lows expressed strong disapproval toward interracial dating, but none of the highs disapproved. On the subject of interracial marriage, none of the highs agreed that it is better for people to marry within their own race, but almost 20% of the lows agreed. On the other hand, nearly 70% of the highs disagreed with the statement, as compared to only about 20% of the lows. Students' responses to these two items indicate that another difference between the groups is that highs are far more accepting of interracial dating and marriage than are lows. Living/Working with minorities. The last two items on the survey explored students' attitudes toward living and working with minorities. Ninety-two percent of the highs indicated that they would enjoy living in a racially diverse neighborhood, whereas only about 35% of the lows expressed this opinion; in addition, 13% of the lows strongly or mostly disagreed with this statement, whereas none of the highs disagreed. All of the high scorers, 100% of them, indicated that they would enjoy teaching in a racially diverse school setting, but less than half of the low scorers expressed a similar opinion. These differing responses clearly show that another major difference between the two groups is that high scorers are much more willing to live in a diverse community and work with a diverse student population. In other words, highs appear to be much more accepting of diversity than are lows. Sensitivity to minority issues. There were two items on the survey, #2 and #4, which provided additional information about students' sensitivity to minority issues. Low scorers were more likely than high scorers (33% compared to 10%) to regard affirmative action as a type of reverse discrimination. Also, high scorers were far more likely than low scorers (63% compared to 7%) to be bothered by the fact that a racial 75 minority person has never been elected President. Thus, a final difference between highs and lows is that highs seem to demonstrate more sensitivity to minority concerns than do lows. In summary, students' responses to the items on the QDI suggest important differences in the racial attitudes, awareness, and sensitivity of high scorers and low scorers. Highs are more likely than lows to regard themselves as unprejudiced, and they seem to hold more positive images of racial minorities than do lows. In addition, highs are more likely than lows to have friendships with people who are racially different than themselves, and they are far more accepting of interracial dating and marriage. The differing responses to the survey items further indicate that high scorers appear to be much more aware of and more knowledgeable about the racism and discrimination that exist in our society, and they also appear to be considerably more sensitive to minority concerns about these problems than are low scorers. Students who scored high are considerably more likely to support the idea of multicultural education than are students who scored low. Finally, highs demonstrate dramatically more positive feelings toward living in a diverse neighborhood and teaching in a diverse school. Although it is possible that all of these differences may actually indicate that highs are better than lows at giving the “politically correct” responses to the survey items, evidence from other quantitative and qualitative data strongly suggest that there are real differences in the racial attitudes of high scorers and low scorers. Volunteer Students One of the major questions of this study was whether or not prospective teachers' racial attitudes are changed by exposure to a multicultural education course. While the end-of-semester administration of the QDI to all students provided interesting information about the differences between high scorers and low scorers, it didn't address the question of whether or not students' racial attitudes changed during the semester. Because the 28 students in the volunteer group were the only ones to whom I 76 administered the QD] both at the beginning of the course and again at the end of the course, I had to answer this question by comparing volunteer students' scores on the beginning-of-semester administration of the QDI with their scores on the end-of-semester administration. Using an analysis of covariance with students' pretest scores on the QDI as the covariate, I obtained the pattern of differences illustrated in Table 4.2 below. Table 4.2 Volunteer Students' Pre and Post Scores on the QD] Students who scored in the middle range on their pretest showed very little change on the posttest. However, the posttest scores of students who initially scored high increased by an average of five points while those of students who initially-scored low decreased by about the same amount. In other words, the students who began the course with the more favorable racial attitudes apparently moved toward having even more favorable attitudes; in contrast, students who began with less favorable racial attitudes apparently moved toward having even less favorable attitudes. Furthermore, because the average score of the highs went up five points while that of the lows went down five points, the difference in the mean attitude scores of high scorers and low scorers actually increased by nearly one full standard deviation. However, since these scores are only for the 28 White volunteers (of which only six were low scorers), they must be interpreted with considerable caution. The data on the volunteer students seem to suggest that there were some changes in student attitudes from the beginning of the semester to the end. However, before this finding can be extrapolated to the entire class, an important question that needs to be addressed is to what extent might the 28 volunteer students be considered representative of the class as a whole? 77 To address this question about the representativeness of my volunteer sample, I conducted several quantitative analyses to determine whether the students who volunteered were different in any important ways (e.g., gender, age, year in college, etc.) from the students who did not volunteer (see Table 4.3). There were only two background characteristics for which I found a significant difference between volunteers and nonvolunteers: age and size of city. Volunteer students were, on average, about two and half years older than nonvolunteers, and they also tended to come from larger cities. The size of city difference appeared to be inconsequential because further analysis revealed no relationship between size of city and students' QDI scores overall. However, the age variable was significantly related to students' QDI scores, with students in the 24— 29 age range scoring about ten points higher than students in the 18-23 age range. (It should be noted that only six students in the entire sample fell into the 24—29 age range, and four of them were in the volunteer group.) As shown in Table 4.3, the QDI scores of volunteers were more titan three points higher than those of nonvolunteers, though this Table 4.3 of Volunteers and Nonvolunteers on Selected Variables Variable Volunteers Nonvolunteers F value difference was not statistically significant. To determine whether the mean score for the volunteer group was higher because of the higher scores of the older students, I compared the QDI scores of the 22 volunteers in the 18-23 age range to those of the 118 nonvolunteers in the same age range. In this analysis, the mean QDI scores were 77.55 78 for volunteers and 74.07 for nonvolunteers, about the same differential as obtained when the older students were included in the analysis. Thus, it appears that, overall, the volunteer students in my sample possessed slightly more favorable racial attitudes than did the nonvolunteers. I do not find it at all surprising that a group of White students who volunteer to participate in a study on diversity with a Black researcher would have somewhat more favorable racial attitudes. While their racial attitudes were slightly more favorable overall, the volunteer group did include a number of low-scoring students who seemed very similar to low-scoring students in the larger class. My analysis of the data also suggests that the volunteer students tended to perform better academically than did the nonvolunteer students. Although there was no difference between volunteers and nonvolunteers on the final exam, volunteers received significantly higher scores on their midterm exam and had significantly better attendance than nonvolunteers (see Table 4.3). In addition, the difference in the final grades of the two groups approached significance. Although one could argue that the students in the volunteer group were not representative of the class as a whole in terms of attendance and academics, I see this difference as more of a plus than a minus. Because the volunteer students had better attendance patterns and appeared to perform better academically, I would expect that they would be more likely to reap the intended benefits of the course than would the nonvolunteer students. In other words, I would expect the volunteer students to give a better indication of the potential effectiveness of the course. In conclusion, regarding the question of whether the volunteer students were representative of students in the class as a whole, the data suggest that they were representative in most respects. On the QDI, the posttest scores of the 28 volunteer students fell in the same range as their pretest scores, with those who scored in the middle range on the pretest tending to receive almost identical scores on the posttest, those who scored high on the pretest tending to score even higher on the posttest, and those who scored low on the pretest tending to score even lower on the posttest. Given that the 79 volunteer students appeared to be representative of the class as a whole, and given that their posttest scores on the QDI tended to fall in the same range (high, middle, or low) as their pretest scores, it seems reasonable to assume that the nonvolunteers who scored in a particular range on the QD] at the end of the semester could have been expected to score similarly at the beginning of the semester, had the QDI been administered to them at that time. Consequently, through the rest of my analysis, I will refer to nonvolunteers who were high scorers or low scorers on the QD] posttest as though they were high scorers or low scorers throughout the entire semester. ABCD Questionnaire The ABCD questionnaire, which was intended to assess their views of teaching and learning, was administered to all CEP 240 students at the beginning and again at the end of the semester. I found that high scorers' and low scorers' responses to the 25 items on the ABCD questionnaire were very similar at the beginning of the semester. In fact, there were only two items on which there was a significant difference between the two groups on the initial administration of the questionnaire (see Table 4.4a, 4.4b, and 4.4c). High scorers were significantly (p < .003) more likely than low scorers to disagree with item #8, which stated, ”The main job of the teacher is to transmit the values of the mainstream American culture.” On the pretest, over 60% of the highs disagreed with this statement, as compared to only 22% of the lows. The second item on which there was a significant difference (p < .02) was #23, which dealt with the acceptability of Black English in the classroom. The responses of the high scorers indicated that they would be more accepting of Black English than would low scorers. Since high scorers, by definition, appeared more accepting of diversity than low scorers, the differences observed on these two items at the beginning of the semester are not surprising. What is surprising, however, is that the difference I observed in students' responses to #23 on the pretest was not present on the posttest. Apparently, over the course of the semester, high scorers and low scorers became more alike in how they responded to this particular item. 80 Table 4.4a Student Responses to ABCD Items #1-14 % Who Strongly % Who Strongly or Chi-square ABCD item & time or Mostly Agree Mostly Disagree Value p Highs Lows Highs Lows 7 1. Teachers should avoid in students by ability or level of performance. I Pre-questionnaire 27.5% 27.3% 20.9% 14.6% 4.668 .5870 | Post-questionnaire 44.9% 32.8% 16.3% 73% 8.316 .2158 2. A lot of my ideas about teachinglnd leamin come from my own experience as a student Pre-questionnaire 90.6% 83.6% 0% 1.8% 3.804 .4332 Post-questionnaire 69.4% 70.9% 4.1% 1.8% 1.472 .9163 3. Teachers should use the same standards in evaluating the work of all students in the class. Pre-questionnaire 44.9% 34.5% 14.2% 23.7% 1 1.270 .0804 Post-questionnaire 12.2% 20.0% 26.5% 25.5% 4.809 .5685 4. It is impractical for teachers to tailor instruction to the unique interests and abilities of different students. Pre-questionnaire 63% 5.4% 56.3% 47.2% 5.444 .4881 Post-questionnaire 4.0% 3.6% 69.4% 45.4% 1 1.821 .0661 5. Students learn best if they have to figure things out for themselves instead of being told or shown. Pre-questionnaire 27.5% 31.5% 14.6% 16.7% 1.774 .9392 Post-questionnaire 26.5% 22.3% 22.5% 27.8% 9.277 .1586 6. When working with slow learners, teachers should focus nearly all their instruction on "minimum comma” objectives. j Prejuestionnaire 0% 1.9% 54.1% 41.5% 3.709 .5920 Post-questionnaire 0% 0% 47.0% 50.9% 2.501 .6444 7. Rguired high school courses should have separate classes for low- and big-achieving students. questionnaire 12.2% 13.0% 36.9% 25.9% 4.770 .5737 Post-quesfionnaire 10.2% 16.4% 42.8% 27.3% 14.034 .0273 8. The main job of the teacher is to transmit the values of the mainstream American culture. Pre-questionnaire 4.2% 3.7% 60.4% 24.1% 20.187 .0026 Post-questionnaire 0.0% 13.0% 71.5% 37.0% 21.497 .0007 9. The main job of the teacher is to encourage students to think and question the world around them. Pre-questionnaire 79.6% 58.2% 2.0% 3.6% 6.614 .3580 Post-questionnaire 81.7% 59.3% 0% 1.9% 8.384 .1363 10. The main job of the teacher is to teach subject matter. Pre-questionnaire 40.8% 25.4% 8.2% 7.2% 6.228 .3980 Post-questionnaire 10.7% 25.4% 12.2% 12.7% 7.479 I .273 11. All students should be taught in English. , ‘ PIE-questionnaire 25.0% 40.0% 20.8% 10.9% 3.928 .6864 Post-questionnaire 12.3% 32.7% 20.4% 7.3% I 14.266 I .0268 p 12. There are some students who can 2122' ly never be m at wriing. Pre-questionnaire 4.0% 3.6% 47.2% 3 . 1% 9.4J33 .1506 ‘ Post-questionnaire 4.1% 7.3% 57.1% 36.3% 6.418 .3780 13. Somepeoplearenaturally abletoor anize their thou ts for writin . 7 Pre-questionnaire 65.3% ism—W “5.6% | .836 .9746 Tom-questionnaire 69.4% 69.1% 2.0% 0% | 5.148 .3981 _ 14. To be good athathematics, you need to have a kind of "mathematical mind” [ Pre-questionnaire 8.1% I 14.5% 40.8% 21.8% 9.669 .1392 | Post-questionnaire 4.2% 21.9% 48.0% 27.3% 11.572 .0722 Table 4.4b 81 Student Responses to ABCD Items #15-22 15. (paraphrased) When students are successful in achieving intended goals or objectives, which do you Highs Lows Chi- Chi- believe is the most frequent source square square 0‘ 8W8? Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Student's home background 22.4% I2§0% 23.6% I 25.9_‘_70_ Student's enthusiasm or perseverance 40.8% I 37.5% 32.7% I 40.75 9.691 2.732 Teacher's attention to students’ 20.4% 14.6% 9.1% 11.1% interests/abilities l6. (paraphrased) When students fail to achieve intended goals or Highs Lows Chi- Chi- objcctives, which do you believe is square square the most frequent sornce of Post failure? Student's home background Student's indifference or perseverance 6.378 Tchr's inattention to students interests/abilities 17. If you were working with low achievers in mathematics, which Highs LOWS Chi- Chi- m of the following would you square square emrhasize most? Pre_ Post Pre Post Pre Post Problem-301mg 9.5% 16.7% E370 I 12.2%; Helping students understand theories 14.3% 12.5% 17.1% 9.7% .884 2.181 Making math class fun for students 66.7% 54.2% 54.3% I 45.2% 18. If you were working with high achievers in mathematics, which Highs LOWS Chi- Chi- mOf the following would you square square emphasize most? Pre Post Pre Post Post Problem-solving 3.6% I 29.2: 32.4 32.1% HelE'gg students understand theories 23.8 37.5% 23.5% 28.6% 2.599 1.150 m math class fun for students 28.6% 12.5 14.7 14.3 19. If you were working with low 7 achievers on learning to write, Highs Lows Chi- Chi- which E; of the following would square square you anvimzc most? Pre Post Pre Post Post . Basic ' and tical skills WWW. 1% 40.6% ‘ Developing turd refining an argument 0%— 7. 7% 2% 12.5 4.637 .644 in writing Having funthroughcanposinghaiku, 39.1% W757 1% 3.1% etc. 20. If you were working with high achievers on leaning to write. Highs Lows Chi- Chi- which mg of the following would square square you unphasizc most? Pre _ Post Pre Post Pre Post Basic ' and tieal trans 439.5. W 2.8% W Developing and refining an argument 61.9% 44% 2.8% 51.7% 4.059 2.424 in writing Having funtinoughcomposinghaiku, 1437915— 8% 83% 6% etc. 82 Table 4.4b (cont) Student Responses to ABCD Items #15-22 21. (paraphrased) Ifyouweretoteach inaclassroomthatincluded students from a variety ofethnic and social backgrormds, circle the three things you would be most likely to do. Highs Pre Post Lows Post Chi- square Chi- square Post Make sure gli_students have the opportunity to uruierstand the subject matter. 57.1% 56.3% 46.3% 40.0% Make sure all students feel good about themselves even if they aren't learning what is in the curriculum. 20.4% 16.7% 27.8% 29.1% Honor and celebrate diversity by having students from different backgrounds share their foods, customsI langu_age, and values. 10.2% 16.7% 11.1% 10.9% 3.693 6.933 22. (paraphrased) Which of the following is closest to your view on the issue of school celebrations (1' Christmas? Lows Pre Post Chi- square Chi- square Post 1) Thereshouldnotbeanycelebration of any religious holiday. 3.8% 7.4% 2) If we celebrate Christian holidays, we also need to celebrate holidays 49. 1% 55.6% of other religions. 4) Om culture is predominantly Christian, and our children should learn these values. 7.5% 3.7% 9.641 10.727 Note: For items #15-22, students selected their responses from a list of six or more choices. In my opinion (which may be different from that of the developers of the items), no one of the choices offered in these items (with the exception of item #22) could properly be considered as the ”right” or even the ”best" response. None of the p values for these eight items approached significance. 83 Table 4.4c Student Responses to ABCD Items #23-25 WAgreeing %-Disagreeing Thi- 23. (paraphrased) Dialects like Black Square p Englisb are fully legitimate mzes- Hi hs Lows Highs Lows Value Pre-Questionnaire ‘_7_6. 1% 717% 23.9% 53% 9.539 .023 Post-Questionnaire 77.3% 60% 22.7% 40% 5.767 .124 24. How would you rate the Opporttmities to learn that minority. Good-Excellent Poor - Fair Chi- special needs, and impoverished Square p Students “filmed in WW Highs Lows Highs Lows Value hr h school? e-Questionnaire 58.7% 75.9% 41.3% 24.1% 3.417 .331 Post-Questionnaire 52.1% =77.0% 47.9% 23.0% _‘7L.994 .046 25. (paraphrased) Do you think minority, special needs, and Yes No Chi- ifmpmisirowormmsmdm’tciminlc m" H gh Lo High Lo Sxilaiiam P 3061111“! cs 0 m- i s ws ' s ws ue Pre-Questionnaire 69.6% 55.5% 19.6% 33.3% 3.541 .315 Post-Questionnaire 95.6% 49.2% 0.0% | 37.0% 34.952 .00000 In my chi-square analysis of students' responses to the end-of—the-semester administration of the ABCD questionnaire, I noted several differences between the responses of highs and lows which suggest that students in the two groups may have reacted quite differently to the course content. On the 25-item questionnaire, there were only five items for which I found a significant difference between highs and lows, plus one item where the difference approached significance. Not surprisingly, these six items were the ones on the questionnaire that were most closely related to the content covered in the course. There was only one item related to content covered in the course-item #1, which dealt with ability grouping--for which the difference between the responses of highs and lows did not approach significance. Since there was a significant difference between highs and lows on the item related to tracking (item #7), I would have expected a similar difference on item #1. I'm not sure why there was no difference on this particular item. I will now briefly discuss each of the six items on which there were differences. First, the initial difference between the two groups on item #8 not only persisted but actually increased in significance (from p < .003 to p < .0007). While the percentage of students who disagreed with item #8 increased for both groups, the percentage who K 84 agreed with the statement increased among lows from 3.7% on the initial questionnaire to 13% on the final questionnaire. In contrast, the percentage of highs who agreed with the statement decreased from 4.2% to 0%. One of the emphases of the course was on accepting diversity and on making the school curriculum more inclusive of other cultures. While high scorers seem to have been influenced in the desired direction by the course, a number of the lows actually appear to have moved in the opposite direction. Apparently, exposure to a course with an express focus on the necessity of acknowledging and celebrating diversity did not alter the belief of many lows that "the values of mainstream American culture" are what all of their students should learn. Although there was no difference on the first questionnaire between the responses of highs and lows to item #7, on the final questionnaire the difference was significant (p < .03). Item #7 stated, ”Required high school courses should have separate classes for low-achieving and high-achieving students.” On the first questionnaire almost 37% of the highs disagreed with this statement, as compared to only 26% of the lows, while on the final questionnaire the percentages were 43% and 27% for highs and lows respectively. The negative effects that tracking can have on diverse learners were addressed in the course readings and lectures. Student responses on the final questionnaire again suggest that exposure to the course content influenced high scorers' existing beliefs about tracking, while the beliefs of the low scorers remained essentially unchanged. A second item for which I found a significant difference (p < .03) on the final questionnaire but not on the initial one was Item #11, which stated that "All students should be taught in English." On the initial questionnaire 40% of the lows and 25% of highs agreed with the statement, while at the end of the course 33% of the lows agreed, in contiast to only 12% of the highs. The topic of bilingual education also received considerable attention in the course readings and lectures. The views of low scorers on this issue were mostly unchanged. Interestingly, however, although the percentage of 85 high scorers who agreed that all students should be taught in English declined by half, there was no corresponding increase in the percentage who disagreed with the statement. This suggests that many of the high scorers shifted to the middle, apparently having become less clear about their position on this issue. A third item where a significant difference appeared on the final questionnaire was item #24, which dealt with how respondents rated the opportunities to learn for minority, special needs, and impoverished students in each respondent's high school. On the first questionnaire 41% of the highs and 24% of the lows rated minorities' opportunities as being poor to fair, but on the final questionnaire 48% of the highs and 23% of the lows gave the same ratings. Apparently, the hi ghs' exposure to the course material led some of them to re-evaluate how minorities and special needs students had been treated in their high schools. On the other hand, the perceptions of the lows appear to have been unchanged. Although the difference between highs and lows on item #4 did not reach significance (p < .07), it is nonetheless worthy of mention. Item #4 asked students whether they thought it was impractical to tailor instruction to meet the needs of different pupils. Among the hi ghs, the percentage of students disagreeing the statement increased from 56% on the pre-questionnaire to 69% on the post-questionnaire. Among the lows, - the percentage of students disagreeing decreased, albeit slightly, from the pre- to the post- questionnaire (from 47% to 45%). The importance of adapting instruction to accommodate the learning needs of diverse learners was a dominant theme in the course through the entire semester. Apparently, the course enabled more of the highs to realize that meeting the unique needs of individual students was practical, whereas not as many of the lows came to this realization. Perhaps the most revealing differences that I found between highs and lows were in their responses to the last item on the questionnaire. Item #25 asked the participants whether or not they believed that minorities, special needs, and impoverished students 86 continue to face unequal opportunities to learn in school. On the initial questionnaire, about 56% of the lows and 70% of the highs expressed the belief that opportunities are unequal, while about 33% of the lows and 20% of the highs asserted that opportunities are equal. These differences were not statistically significant. On the final questionnaire, however, 59% of the lows and 96% of the highs believed that opportunities were unequal. Quite surprisingly, the lows who still believed that opportunities are equal actually increased to 37%; in contrast, none (0%) of the highs continued to believe that opportunities are equal. These differences on the final questionnaire were highly significant (p < .00000). Explanation of the inequalities that have existed in the educational system for minorities and special needs students was a major focus throughout the course, one that was reiterated again and again in a variety of ways through readings, lectures, and lab section discussions (see Appendix A). Considering that this issue was given such extensive treatment throughout the semester and that high scorers and low scorers were all exposed to the same information, the fact that more than one-third of the lows still held to their belief that everyone has equal opportunities to learn in school while none of the highs retained this belief seems to support my hypothesis that students' racial attitudes mediate what they learn from a course on diversity. My examination of the group gain scores for each of the six previously-discussed items on the final questionnaire seems to provide further support for my hypothesis. If students' racial attitudes are related to what they appear to learn from the course, one would expect students with more favorable attitudes (high scorers) to demonstrate more change in the desired direction than those with less favorable attitudes (low scorers). As illustrated in Table 4.5, on each of the six items where I found the greatest difference between highs and lows on the final questionnaire, the views of students who entered the course with the most favorable racial attitudes did, in fact, evidence more movement in the desired direction than did students who entered with less favorable attitudes. In fact, 87 the views of students with less favorable racial attitudes appeared to have been little affected by the course content and demonstrated little or no positive change over the SCIDCSICI'. Table 4.5 Change Scores for Highs and Lows on Selected ABCD Items Low ABCD Item &Time IScoirers IChange Scorers Change I Fvalue p 4. It rs impractical for teachers to tailor instruction to the unique interests ad abilities of different students. Pre-questionnaire 5.354 5. 127 Post-questionnaire 5.979 .625 5.327 .200 4.138 .018 7. Required high school courses should have separate classes for low- and high-achievin students. Pre-questionnaire I 4.809 4.451 Post-questionnaire I 5.234 .425 4.216 -.235 4.308 I .015 8. The mainjob of the teacher is to transmit the values of the mainstream American culture. Pre-questionnaire 5.596 4. 529 Post-questionnaire 5.957 .361 4.686 . 15 7 .071 .0 11. All students shouldbetaughtinEnglish __ Pre-questionnaire I 3.936 I | 3.327 I Post-questionnaire | 4.234 | .298 | 3.291 -.036 1.915 |.151 24. How would you rate theopportunities to learn that minority, special needs. and impoverished students experienced in your high school? Pre-questionnaire 2.244 1.9% I fl l Post-questionnaire 2.366 .122 1.896 I 483 3.43 |.035 25. (paraphrased) Do you think minority, special needs, and impoverished students continue to face unemial opporttmities to learn? Pre-questionnaire I 2. 93—3 I r | 2.45 | I Post-questionnairel 3.600 | .66 | 2.396 | -.057 I 16.36 |.000 Course Records At the conclusion of the course, I analyzed the course records of all students (i.e., attendance, exam grades, and course grades) to determine whether there were any important differences in the attendance or academic performance of high students and low students. While students' responses to some of the items on the ABCD questionnaire indicate that the course appeared to influence the views of highs considerably more than those of lows, my examination of students' exam scores suggest that there was no difference between the two groups in terms of their attendance and academic performance. Specifically, my analysis of the exam scores revealed that highs and lows did not differ in their performance on the midterm and final (See Table 4.6). Although 88 highs did receive the highest scores on the two exams and the lows received the lowest scores, the difference between the groups was not statistically significant. The midterm and final each consisted of 40 multiple choice questions worth 80 points and 2-5 essay questions worth a total of 20 points. Both exams drew heavily on the factual information presented in the readings and lectures, and students' essay responses also had to be supported with specific references to the readings and lectures. Table 4.6 Mean Scores for Attendance, Midterm Exam, Final Exam, and Final Grade While exam scores are certainly not the most reliable indicator of student learning, if one were to assume for a moment that these two exams do provide some indication of what the students may have Ieamed in the course, it would appear that lows may have learned about as much in the course as did highs, a situation, if it were in fact true, that would be contrary to what has been reported in earlier studies (Bennett, Niggle, & Stage, 1990; Ross & Smith, 1992). My quantitative analysis of course records further revealed no difference in the attendance patterns of lows and highs. In fact, highs actually missed classes more frequently than did lows. This would seem to contradict the assumption that those with less favorable attitudes toward minorities are likely to express resistance to multicultural course content by missing classes more frequently. Highs and lows also did not differ on the final grades they received for the course. Qualitative Data I collected qualitative data only for the 28 volunteer students. As explained in the previous chapter, I selected 14 of these 28 volunteers (seven with high QDI scores and seven with low QDI scores) to be my target students. I then interviewed these target 89 students at the beginning and at the end of the course, and also at approximately two- week intervals throughout the semester. In this section I will report first the patterns that I observed in my analysis of the pre- and post-interviews with the target students. Then I will present a close-up look at three selected target students, drawing on data from the intervening interviews, as well as from the pre- and post-interviews. Target Students One of my research questions focused on how students with more favorable racial attitudes might be similar to and different from students with less favorable attitudes. While this question was answered to some extent in my eariier discussion of the QDI responses of high scorers and low scorers, my analysis of the pre- and post-interviews with the fourteen target students (see Table 4.7) revealed some additional interesting similarities and differences between highs and lows in several broad areas: students' background experiences, their beliefs about the education of minorities, the apparent impact of the course on the students, and their reactions to being interviewed. Table 4.7 Target Students _lTi—Sc ghorers Low orers Cindy Bill Ernest Bob Judy Claire Karen Jan Kathy Jill Terri Joy Tina Kate Bac und ri ces During the first interview (see Appendix D), the target students were asked a number of questions about their background and their previous interracial experiences, in addition to questions about diversity issues. In analyzing their responses to these 90 questions for patterns of similarity and difference between highs and lows, I was able to identify several categories of background experiences which appeared to be important for understanding the two groups of students. These categories were community characteristics, friendships with minorities, and awareness of racism and discrimination. Although the data supporting these categories were drawn primarily from the first interview, a couple of students made relevant comments in the final interview which were also considered in my analysis. Community characteristics. Eleven of the 14 target students (six lows and five highs) came from communities that were at least 95% White, according to 1990 census data. In most cases, therefore, the schools that the students attended were not very diverse racially. One of the low students and two of the high students came from three different small cities that were each about 80-85% White and from 10-15% Black. However, the low student, because of housing patterns in the 'city, attended a high school that was virtually all White. The two high students, on the other hand, each attended a high school that was very racially diverse. The fact that most of the low-scoring students, as well as most of the high-scoring students, came from communities that were very similar in their racial composition would seem to suggest that this factor alone is not a reliable predictor of students’ racial attitudes. Friendships with racial minorities. Although all students reported having at least a few racial minorities in attendance at their high schools, highs reported having more friendships with individuals from different racial groups than did lows, and they also reported higher quality friendships. During the first interview, students were asked to think of friendship as having three levels: Level Three being their closest friends, those whom they confide in and spend a lot of time with; Level Two being their casual friends, those whom they do things with or go out with owasionally; and Level One being their acquaintances. Only two of the low students reported having a Level Three (close) friend from a different racial group (and coincidentally, in each case the friend was a Native 91 American). The other five low students reported having no Level Three friendships with individuals from a different racial group, and four of these five also reported no Level Two friendships either. In contrast, only one of the high students indicated that he had no close friendship with a person of a different race. (This student did have a close Arab friend, but Arabs are generally considered to be of the White race.) The six remaining high students all reported having at least one interracial friendship at Level Three and numerous friendships at Level Two. These data parallel and would seem to corroborate the pattern I observed in students' responses to the QDI, where highs were significantly more likely than lows to report having a friendship network that was racially-mixed. Although the data I collected do not enable me to determine whether highs have more interracial friendships because they have more favorable racial attitudes or whether they have more favorable racial attitudes because they have more interracial friendships, they do suggest that there is a strong relationship between the two variables. Awareness of racism and discrimination. Despite the similarities in the racial makeup of their communities and schools, at the beginning of the course, highs showed greater awareness than lows of racial prejudice and discrimination in their hometowns and in the schools they attended. For several of the low students, prejudice and discrimination were things that occurred elsewhere, but in their communities and in their schools they contended that everyone was treated the same, that a person's race didn't matter. Jill claimed that "in my school, it didn't make any difference what you were or who you were. It didn't make any difference” (Jill 201-199,201). Similarly, Kate contended that all students in her school were treated the same. I don't feel like anyone was like treated different just because they think that, like, guys are better in math and science than girls. That was never the casein my schools. I thought that everyone was taught the same, and if you needed help, then they were there to help you, but they didn't look upon you as like being dumb if you did need help. So you don't think minorities have unequal opportunities? That's the same, too. Our school was, like, right by Detroit. We had some Black kids and we had some White kids and we had Chinese and Latinos, and we had all races and everything so that was the same for that, too. I mean, everyone's put together and no one's segregated. [Kate 207-89, 93] 92 Bill echoed these sentiments in his response to a question about the reasons for lower minority school achievement: . . . it's hard to say discrimination, because I don't see how a teacher could, like, openly discriminate against a student, so I don't really know. You haven't seen evidence of any teacher discrimination against students of any type? No, never. [Bill 131-152,154] Claire firmly believed that everyone has equal opportunities and that discrimination is not a problem either in schools or in society. When asked to explain the lower school achievement of minorities, she responded, "I don't think it's because they've been taught less or something to that effect 'cause, like I said before, everybody's got the same opportunity, I believe" [Claire 211-84]. While the three other low students acknowledged that racism and discrimination might be problems for racial minorities, their beliefs in this regard tended to be both tenuous and tentative. Bob, for instance, averred that racism and discrimination has handicapped racial minorities in this society and that it has also served to depress minority school achievement. At the same time, however, he expressed the contradictory notion that minorities have equal opportunities to learn, except in certain places where schools may be inadequately funded, and regarding funding he went on to say, "the opportunities may not have been equal, but I've seen that minorities and special needs do have a lot of opportunities now to go to college, and there's financial support and special--, so I think that's equaling it out” [Bob 127-140]. Though acknowledging the historical presence of racism and discrimination, he seemed to underestimate the depth and breadth of its continuing impact. Asked to explain the lower school achievement of minority pupils, Jan suggested that the attitudes of teachers and administrators might serve to inhibit minority achievement. She commented, "Maybe they think that if you're a minority, you can't do as well" [Jan 128-168]. She admitted, however, that she was merely speculating, and she did not appear very confident in this belief. Also addressing the question of lower minority achievement, Joy posited that the prejudice of teachers 93 might be a factor. ”I think a lot of teachers--, I don't know if it's like consciously or not, but I think they gravitate more to the White children because they're the same as them” [Joy 128-85]. Interestingly, Joy saw the prejudice as a predilection for White children rather than as an aversion to minority children. White teachers gravitate to White children because ”they're the same as them," suggesting that their doing so is both natural and understandable. Joy seemed to attempt to further soften the image of the teachers' prejudice with her assertion that it was ”Not conscious prejudice. I think it's unconscious. I don't think a teacher would on purpose single a--, like, gravitate to the White people” [Joy 128-87]. While she acknowledged the existence of prejudice, she did not appear to be too comfortable with it, nor did she show a good understanding of its true nature. In contrast to the low students, the high students generally seemed to be much more cognizant of the problems of prejudice and discrimination in their schools and communities. Tina, for instance, demonstrated not only an awareness of the differential treatment that minority students received from teachers but also an understanding of the depth and breadth of the problem of racial prejudice. When asked about how minorities were treated in school, she responded, They get talked down to. It's not the material; it's the way it's presented, and that again is based on just observation, I guess really, but it seems like they're talked down to, just like, 'Well, you're not smart enough to understand if I do this quick and fast in normal language, so I've gotta be nice and sweet.” [Tina 127-181] A short while later our conversation proceeded as follows: So, if the teachers weren't prejudiced, then the minorities and poor students would have equal opportunities? Or more equal, yeah. Probably not still equal because our society--, tlrat'll be nine gabillion years down the road, just because of society, but I think if teachers are less prejudiced, the students will have more opportunities. What does society have to do with it? Because that comes to where the prejudice comes in. Teachers aren't gonna not be prejudiced for a while unless they've been raised to not be prejudiced, and in our society that's very, very difficult to do. Why? Just because it is. I mean, no matter how--, even my prejudices that I have, my family has tried very, very hard to make us non-prejudiced. I mean, they have, and society makes it very difficult with the stereotypes that society puts out and that you're in everyday to not have some biases. [Tina 127-201,203,205] 94 Karen was also cognizant of the differential treatment that minority students received from teachers. . . . part of our problem is that there are teachers that are unfair, in general, in the classroom to minorities. What are you basing that on? I've had teachers that have been unfair to minorities that-- But I thought there weren't minorities at your school? There were, actually. There were three Black children in my class, and I can say that the teachers sometimes were not fair to them. I mean, you could see-- Can you give me an example? Well, there's a lot of prejudice, and sometimes there are prejudiced statements made and, a lotta times they weren't called on, and it still exists; I mean, it hasn't gone away. [Karen 128-166,168,170,174] Judy spoke of culturally biased tests and prejudiced teachers as being major contributing factors to the lower achievement of minority students, and Kathy also expressed the belief that prejudice was a factor. Two of the high students reported that they had not personally witnessed evidence of racism and discrimination in their high school, but both students were aware of their presence in the larger community. Cindy, first of all, commented that . . . it was not in my school, but I have experienced it at the job I worked at. How so? Our staff that I worked with was—, I wouldn't say predominately White 'cause I don't think it was. I think it probably was just about equal, I would say, and you could just see how some of the workers that were White would treat some of the Black customers or the way some of the Black workers would treat some of the White customers, you know, but I'm just saying in my school, there wasn't that kind of discrimination. From experiencin' that at my job, you know, I'm just assuming that it would be at other schools since there is so much of it. I would assume there is some at other schools. [Cindy l31-120,122] Terri's comment that she was not aware of racism and discrimination in her high school was rather surprising to me. She attended a high school which she described as being fifty percent minority, and her remarks elsewhere in the first interview, as well as in the final interview, clearly indicate that she was one of the students in the class who were most experienced with and knowledgeable about racism and discrimination. For example, in the first interview Terri observed, My friends are from all different backgrounds, all different races, and when I sit in the class and when we talk in our lab, I hear people saying, "I've never met anyone that's different than me. My school was all White. My school was all — _ 95 this.” Well, my school was very diverse. I Ieamed all that--, I mean, I just think that a lot of the experiences I've had and knowing what to do and what not to do, I've learned definitely not to stereotype people, and I see so many people doing it here. It's because they don't know at all, and I think that I've been educated well enough. I mean, I just think that I have had enough experiences right now that a lot of these things we read about, a lot of these things I have already known, I've already experienced, but there's a lot of people in the class that are looking at this for the first time, and they're just thinking, "Oh, my gol." It's so new to them, but to me it's not new at all. [Terri 131-60] During her final interview Terri offered a more pointed statement of her awareness: I think that it's very apparent and there are still a lot of people who do have racist beliefs, and there are people who are discriminated against. I do think it's getting better. I mean, I do think that people are waking up and finally, actually, like younger generations, are seeing it, but I think that it is something that is very alive today. [Terri 426121] Only one of the high students, Ernest, reported no awareness of racial prejudice in his high school or community, although at one point he did hypothesize that maybe White teachers give less attention to and expect less from Black students. Although several low-scoring students claimed that racism and discrimination were not problems in their home communities, given the fact that their communities were so similar to those of hi gh-scoring students in terms of their racial composition, what seems more likely-and the data reported above would appear to bear this out--is that high students had a greater awareness of and sensitivity to instances of racial minorities’ receiving differential treatment. Also, while highs and lows alike generally reported some degree of awareness of racism and discrimination in the larger society beyond their home communities, highs tended to display a greater breadth and depth of understanding of these problems, as illustrated in the quotations cited earlier in this section. I think again that this may well relate back to the fact that highs tended to bring to the course a broader base of interracial experiences. It is possible that having this broader base enabled them to develop broader perspectives on various issues of diversity. Beliefs about Qe flucation of Mingrities Minorities' opportunities to learn. At the beginning of the semester only three of the seven low students expressed the belief that minority pupils have unequal 96 opportunities to learn in school, and this number was unchanged at the end of the semester. On the other hand, six of the seven high students initially believed that opportunities were unequal, and all seven expressed this belief at the end of the course. The low students' belief that minority pupils had equal opportunities to learn seemed to be based exclusively on what they themselves had seen and experienced during their own schooling. For example, Bill asserted that ”I've never been in a situation where I've seen people were discriminated against or anything like that (Bill 131-118),” and Claire explained her belief that opportunities were equal by saying, I don't believe that these people that are listed--the minorities, special needs, girls, etc.»face unequal opportunities. I'm a girl. My major is math, sol kind of like prove it wrong in a sense there. If somebody has a will, then they'll go as far as they want to, and whether a person does go far, it's not they don't have the opportunity. I just believe it's because they don't want to. [Claire 211-70] When asked whether she thought minorities had equal opportunities to learn in school, Kate responded, ”I think they do 'cause, I mean, they're all in the same classes" [Kate 207-91]. She seemed to believe that since minority pupils were in the same classrooms as majority pupils, then they must have equal opportunities to learn. Another low student, Bob, also expressed this belief and provided the following rationale for it: . . . if you have a teacher that's teaching to a class and, you know, it's half Whites and half people of other cultures or Black, and they're giving out the information. I guess everybody has an equal opportunity to study and read their books and take in the information as anyone else, and everybody has equal opportunity to search out that education. [Bob 127-146] The comments of these low students suggest that they were oblivious to many subtle ways in which minorities experience inequalities in the American educational system. In addition, although several of the low students admitted the possibility that minority students might face unequal opportunities in other schools (but not in their schools), they relied primarily on their perceptions of their own experiences. In other words, they placed more credence in their own personal experiences than in the reported experiences of others. Specifically, Bill commented that he had never seen people discriminated against, Claire felt that because she was a girl and she hadn't experienced discrimination, 97 then it couldn't be true that girls were discriminated against, and Kate believed that minorities had equal opportunities to learn because she had not seen unequal opportunities at her school. Only a couple of the low students seemed to acknowledge the possibility that their experiences (and their interpretation of those experiences) may have been limited. In contrast, the high students, while also drawing primarily on their personal schooling experiences, were nevertheless able to identify specific ways in which minorities' educational opportunities were not equal. For example, one student commented, . . .yeah, I believe people do face unequal opportunities. I'm not saying that about everybody. I'm saying, you know, it does happen. What are some examples of these unequal opportunities that you're thinking of? For one, how in an AP course is there all White kids and then in a lower class all Black kids? I don't understand, you know. What's the explanation for that? [Cindy 506-1 12,114] ‘ And another student identified other ways that minorities may face unequal opportunities to learn. A lot of tests that measure your competency sometimes don't ask questions that are fair. I know they are revising the SAT. I've read a lot about that, but I remember there was a big controversy about that. [Karen 128-146] There's questions that are partial to--, I can't think of a specific example right offhand, but there are questions that are partial to White males, and, you know, students that are of color or European or Asian don't have the same experiences and couldn't answer the questions as easily. [Karen 128-152] UK, are there any other ways or reasons that these groups have unequal opportunities to learn? Well, I think a lot of schools that need funding, such as Benton Harbor, is mainly minority, and I don't think that that's fair. There's a lot of violence problems there, and I think if they had more f unding--, I mean, that's not fair to everyone who goes to that school. [Karen 128-156] Another way that high students differed from lows was in their willingness to believe that unequal opportunities exist even though they had not personally witnessed them. I've only had experiences with the female part, but I think it holds true for all groups. Math and science are considered the two hardest subjects. I mean, in general, and I really think that people who are impoverished or minorities or female really get talked down to when it comes to those subjects. I don't know why. I don't like it, but I've noticed it, I mean, personally, that they're not taken as seriously, they're not taken as, well, they can do this on the high level that I'm teaching it because they can, period. Oooh! It drives me nuts. [Tina 127-163] 98 Kathy showed a similar willingness to believe that minorities' opportunities to learn were unequal. I think that they probably are not pushed as hard as everyone else. Why not? I don't know. Because of stereotypes. [Kathy 202-176, 178] . . . [minorities] get that attitude, maybe not as directly, but in the way the teacher treats 'em and in the way they look at 'em and stuff like that, and in the way they act towards them. . . . This difi'erent treatment that you're saying minorities get, what makes you think that's true?) What makes me think that's true? Just from what we talked about in this class, actually. I can't honestly remember seeing that. But you still believe it? Yeah, just because I see what happens everywhere else. [Kathy 202-182,]84] Even though she had not seen actual evidence of unequal treatment in school, based on what she had seen elsewhere, she readily believed that it also occurs in schools. In addition, the high students demonstrated more awareness/knowledge of the educational disparities beyond the confines of their particular school system, as the following exchange with Judy indicates: I don't think they have equal opportunities to learn. 0K, what makes you think that? Well, you know, just the studies they've done about the SAT and the ACT. I believe it's changing and getting better, but I believe that typical education in America is geared towards the White male, because when they started teaching in the United States, that was what--, that's who they were teaching. They didn't care about the girls. They didn't care about any of the races or cultures. All they cared about was the White males. . . . [Judy 204-100, 104] In some of her comments Tina also demonstrated an understanding that the problem is a national one. Do you think that's minority, special needs, poor students have unequal opportunities to learn in schools? Yes, I do based on teachers' personal prejudices towards whatever the child may have been categorized as. The American school system is set up primarily for the middle class White student--male, and if you're not that level, then you're automatically gonna be biased against. The tests are gonna be White-biased, the texts are gonna be White-biased, and the way the teachers teach is gonna be White-biased. Middle-class White- biased. Therefore, that automatically puts that student at a disadvantage, and puts them at a lower level of being able to achieve what they want to achieve. They're gonna have to work twice as hard to get there. [Tina 422-160] 99 As a final example, Terri recognized and was distressed by the inequities experienced by minority students in our inner cities. . . . in a lot of the readings that we had to do, like in my 188 last semester, we read a lot about inner-city Chicago, inner-city New York. . . . kids were going to school in an old roller skating rink, and I mean, just like all these things that happened to these people, and sometimes because they are of lower class and because they don't have enough money and then again because of their background, the color of their skin, they're getting put in these schools that--, because the predominantly White parents don't want them in their school, and to me that is so ridiculous because, I mean, I just don't even see how people could do that. [Terri 131-106] Explanation of lower minority achievement. In the first interview, when students were asked to explain why most minority students don't do as well in school as most White students, I observed some interesting similarities and differences in the explanations that they offered. Table 4.8 offers a summary of the explanations that the target students offered for the lower school achievement of minority students. (The numbers in parentheses indicate how many of the seven students in group mentioned a particular response.) Explanations which related to characteristics within the individual or within the individual's home life I classified as personal (or trait) explanations. Explanations which related to school factors or other factors outside the individual I classified as situational explanations (Cushner, McClelland, & Safford, 1992). Whether prospective teachers attribute lower minority achievement primarily to personal factors or . primarily to situational factors is an important consideration because their attributions are likely to determine how they will respond to the problem. As Table 4.8 illustrates, low students were considerably more likely to offer personal explanations than were high students, and conversely, lows were less likely to offer situational explanations. In fact, situational explanations were offered only five times by the low students, and three of those explanations came from the same student. Clearly, therefore, the lows were inclined to view the causes of lower minority school achievement as lying in the students or in their families. Kate, for example, identified students' personal motivation and their home life as the primary explanations for lower minority achievement. 100 Table 4.8 Explanations of Lower Minority School Achievement Low Students High Students (5) home background (4) home background (2) low self -expectations (2) personal motivation Personal Explanations (2) heredity (1) being/feeling different (1) Black culture (1) personal motivation (2) teacher attitudes (3) teacher attitudes Situational Explanations ( 1) teacher expectations (2) discrimination (1) test bias (2) school quality (1) school quality (1) teacher expectations (1) test bias I guess it's on their own personal, like, striving for doing the best I mean, maybe there's a study showing that Whites strive harder or--- . . . . Maybe home life, or I don't know. I mean, if kids are poor, maybe they just don't have like the life at home that parents are like pushing you to do better and maybe they just don't care. Maybe they see their parents as failing and themselves being like, well, if my parents failed, then why should I even try. I might not get anywhere in life. There's no use in trying. I mean, kids might have that attitude. [Kate 207- 101,105] Claire likewise partly attributed the lower achievement to the students' home life, but she also attributed it partly to heredity. I think a lot of it has to do with home background. They're not intending to bring problems to school with them, but if they're constantly on their mind at home, they're going to affect their ways in class. . . . I don't know, maybe it's hereditary and the genes don't carry through to cany a lot of information. [Claire 204-7684] Finally, Bob offered low expectations-on the part of teachers, society, and the students themselves--as an explanation, and he also suggested that Black culture played a role in low school achievement. I think a possibility could be that the effect of racism and prejudice towards people, you know, lowers your expectations for yourself, so it's kinda like the self-fulfilling prophecy, you know, that's put out throughout society, you know, on television, so that perhaps they' re not expected by teachers or society to do as well and therefore they don't. Also, I can see having a big effect rs that in Black culture today education isn't stressed as much as looks and material things like that, so they're, you know, not putting as much effort into education or desiring to and therefore maybe possibly reducing test scores and their performance. [Bob 127-152] 101 Despite his mention of low societal expectations, Bob still seems to believe that most of the explanation is to be found within the students themselves, or at least within their culture. Although many of the high students also made reference to personal (trait) factors in their explanations, as a group they seemed to be more aware of the various situational factors that have been associated with lower minority achievement. Whereas only three of the low students made any reference to situational factors in their explanations, all but one of the high students did so in their explanations. Tina, for example, cited teacher attitudes and school quality as primary factors. I think for one, teachers probably spend less time. It goes back to the stereotypes that, well, this person is poor, this person is a certain minority, and they're not as smart or whatever. That goes back to that, but I also think that kids who are poor--, if it's a poorer community, they're gonna have poorer quality schools 'cause there's not as much money for funding, for books, for whatever, and so they're gonna get a poorer education, and I mean, they're not gonna perform as well if they take a standardized test because they haven't had the background for it 'cause there's not the money for the background for it. I think that's the big one. [Tina 127-213] Cindy commented that “the first thing that comes to mind to me would probably be discrimination. You know, the White students are given more attention by the teachers” [Cindy 131-134]. Finally, Karen attributed the lower achievement to discrimination and home background factors. They aren't given as much attention. By whom? By teachers. By society. A lot of minorities are single-parent families, more so than White children, and I think that the single-parent families can't devote as much time to their children as they need to, and that creates a problem. [Karen 128-176,178] Teaching minority students. During the final interview I asked all of the target students the following question: ”Given the fact that poor minority students don't do as well in school as White students, if you were teaching poor minority students, would you have to do anything differently to be effective with them?" Although on this question the responses of highs and lows were not as sharply dissimilar as on other questions, some differences did emerge. First of all, four of the low students expressed the belief that the 102 minority students should receive the same instruction as everyone else. For example, in response to the above question, Bill replied, "I don't think so. Just treat everyone the same" [Bill 428-131]. Similarly, Jill responded, "I would try the same thing with everybody. I don't see any diff erence" [Jill 426—138]. Another of the low students, Claire, commented, . . . as far as them being minority, I think again it’s just coincidental because there’s, again, White people that, you know, don’t do as well either. Not everybody can be on top. So you wouldn ’t see yourself doing anything dtflerently with minorin low achievers than you would with White low achievers? No. [Claire 429-74,?6] In Claire's opinion, the fact that minority students are more likely to be low achievers than White students is merely ”coincidental.” Consequently, she sees no reason to teach minority students any differently. Another low student, Joy, acknowledged that minority students might require extra attention, but she felt that giving some students extra time would be inequitable to the others. So you 'd give the same attention to the ones who aren't achieving as you would to those who are achieving? Um hmm. I would try to. But if they aren't achieving, don 't they need more attention? They do, but then you're cheating the children who are excelling, you know, if you're taking away their time and you're giving more to these children. [Joy 428-13 1,133] Only one of the low students, Bob, made a statement suggesting that he might approach the teaching of minority students somewhat differently. I would have to find out what level they are at and what their things are that they relate to, I guess, and what their interests are. And bring some of their culture into it so they feel proud about that. [Bob 426-140] Thus, for most of the low students, the fact that minorities' school performance has historically been lower than that of White students would have little, if any, impact on how they would go about teaching their minority students. Although one of the high students , Ernest, also expressed the belief that all students should be treated the same, he appeared to be a bit more sensitive to the fact that minority students might have difierent needs. He asserted, 103 Obviously I'd have to change a little bit if a student's needs are different. But on the whole, as far as one class versus another, I'd expect the same out of both groups. I guess and I wouldn't change my style. Like I wouldn't be easier on one class than the other just because they have it tougher, I guess. [Ernest 425-1 14] In contrast to the low students, most of the high students seemed to believe that in order to address the historical disparities in minority school achievement, they would have to do something different for minority students in their classrooms. High students appeared to be much less likely to believe that all students should be taught the same. For example, one student responded, Well, I think all kids learn differently, so I think to teach everybody in your class effectively, you're gonna have to use different ways of teaching. I think to be effective you just use a lot of different teaching techniques and know where your students are, 'cause if you know where your students are and what the problems are, then you can compensate for it. I mean, I can't say that I know how I would have to teach differently, you know, to get the same results with minority students as non-minority students but--- [Judy 425-158] Terri offered a response that was very much like Judy's. I would probably just teach how it's going to be more productive to them. I mean, maybe if they haven't got the attention that they've needed from teachers whatever, or they may be behind or just something like that, I think I would have to, you know, find what it is that's gonna have them, you know, find hope again in the school system, or find hope in learning, and just kind of go from there. I don't really know anything too specific. [Terri 426-83] Although she expresses it less clearly than the previous two individuals, Cindy seemed to hold a similar idea about teaching minority students. I was gonna say, you know, not to treat anybody differently but then again, I know we had talked about in class that teaching fairly--, or equally equals fair or something. I can't remember exactly what it was, but about just because you're teaching them fair, doesn't mean it's equally, or whatever that was. That you treat them and teach them to what is fair to them. To their learning ability. [Cindy 506-88] Explaining what she would do with minority students, Karen said that she would do "probably whatever needs to be done” [Karen 427-79]. Finally, Tina also indicated that she would endeavor to identify and then address the particular needs of each individual. If you were teaching poor minority students, what kinds of things would you have to do difi’erently, to be eflective with them? I don't know because it depends on what the problem is. . . . That's probably where I would get stuck because I would wanta talk to every single kid that's 104 having problems in my classes and find out what was going on, why they were having problems, as far as what I could do to help them, blah, blah, blah-~which is never gonna happen because there's not enough hours in a day. (Brief pause) I really don't know. It would really depend on the type of school, the situation, what was going on with the kids, if it was a behavioral problem, if it was a motivational problem. [Tina 422- 140] In analyzing the target students' responses to the question about teaching minority students, I also observed an interesting but somewhat disturbing similarity between the highs and lows. As illustrated most cleariy in the comments of Judy and Terri above, none of the students seemed to have any clear ideas about what they could do to bolster the achievement of minority students, though several did talk about the necessity of taking pupils' individual needs and differences into account. This circumstance is not completely surprising, given the fact that these students have not yet begun their teacher education courses. However, during their semester in CEP 240, students were exposed not only to the various ways in which common school structures and practices may serve to depress minority student achievement, but also to a number of strategies for working effectively with pupils who are culturally different, including using cooperative learning groups, diversifying the curriculum, giving some students more time or help to complete their work, and valuing the experiences that the students bring to the learning situation. Virtually none of these ideas appeared in their responses. Apparently, most of the target students did not make the connection between what they had learned in the course and how that learning could be applied in their actual teaching of minorities. This findings seems to be consistent with Paine's (1990) finding that prospective teachers ”had the greatest difficulty analyzing and being explicit about the pedagogical implications of diversity' (p. 11). Or perhaps, also consistent with what Paine reported, when these students think about teaching minorities, they conceive only of teaching individual children and not groups. Immt of @urse Most important leamings. During the final interview the target students were asked to identify the three most important things that they felt they had Ieamed from the 105 course. My examination of the students' answers to this question revealed no clear differences between the responses of highs and lows; the two groups of students seemed to report learning the same types of things. In particular, all of the students reported increased awareness of some diversity issues. Table 4.9 briefly summarizes the leamings identified by each student and illustrates that highs and lows reported similar types of gains in knowledge and awareness. While self -reports cannot be regarded as reliable measures of students' knowledge growth, they do suggest that students' perceptions of what they had learned were very much alike for highs and for lows. Although highs' and lows' perceptions of what they learned from the course appeared to be very similar, their perceptions of what they learned about themselves were quite different. Four of the low students indicated that they had Ieamed nothing about themselves from the course, whereas all seven of the highs reported some increase in their self -awareness or self-understanding. One of the purposes of the course was to have students examine their own attitudes and beliefs in light of the information presented during the semester; apparently, high students engaged in this kind of self-examination moreso than low students. For example, in response to the question "Have you learned any important things about yourself from this class?” Bill, one of the low students, replied, "Not really” [Bill 428-26]. Jill offered a similar response: ”No. Not for myself. It's just that I'm just learning more stuff for a teacher. Not really about myself. I pretty much knew" [Jill 426-14]. In her mind, the content that she had learned related to her as a teacher but not as a person. A third low student, Jan, was unaware of or just unable to verbalize whether she had Ieamed anything about herself. She told me, ”I probably did but either I can't think of it or I can't put it into words. Right now, I don't know" [Jan 429-34]. Claire indicated that the class had not given her any new insights into herself, only reinforced something that she already knew. A lot of it emphasized about how I'm willing to give everybody a chance, you know, whether they're Black or White or tall or short whatever, and it just kind of emphasized that that's really important; it wasn't really anything new. [Claire 429-16] 106 Students' responses to the Quick Discrimination Index and the ABCD questionnaire indicated that the low students were the ones who had the most distance to travel in terms of developing positive attitudes toward minorities, but the comments of these four students suggest that the lows may have been the ones who were least affected personally by the course content Although it is possible, as may have been the case for Jan, that these students were either unaware of or unable to verbalize what they had learned about themselves, I think it is more likely that they were not significantly affected in a personal manner by the course. If they had been affected, why would they be any less aware or less able than the high students to verbalize their new learrrings about themselves? Another possibility, of course, is that for some students the personal impact of the course may not have become apparent to them until weeks or perhaps months afterwards. In contrast to these four low students, all of the high students identified one or more ways in which their self awareness or self -understanding had been expanded as a result of the course. Karen, for example, realized that she needed to think more about her own values. Have you learned any important things about yourself in this course? Pretty much that a lot of my values are values that my parents had and you made me think about them a little bit more. So it's gonna make me wanta go out there and try to decide things more on my own, you know. I'm gonna wanta look things up now before I make a decision. So you learned to examine your own values? Yeah, exactly. [Karen 427-8,10] Terri reported that, in addition to reaffirrning or changing some of the beliefs that she began the course with, she became aware that she seemed to be more tolerant than many of her peers. I think that I've expanded on a lot of things that I believed in before and now I either believe in them really more or my mind has been changed about different things. I've found that I seem to be a little more tolerant than a lot of people. [Terri 426-12] Tina also became aware of the fact that she was more accepting of diversity than were many of her classmates. Table 4.9 Most Important Learnings Low Students 107 Hiih Students Bill. 1. learning styles 2. Openedmindtomorepeopleandtowhatpeople are thinking 3. identifying children with learningdisabilities Cindy 1. to be more open-minded with diversity issues 2. accepting people for whatever they are and not discriminating m l. needtobeawareofthediversityinthe classroom and all the things diversity requires 2. involve students 3. looking at the person before the label Ernest a l. more minority awareness 2. reinforcement of a lot of stuff he already knew 3. some issues are big Clare 1. never assume anything about a child's ability 2. have a complete open mind and give everybody My 1. about other people's attitudes 2. different ways educators are trying to deal with l. the differences that people can have 2. laws 3. more about learning disabilities a chance diversity 3. diversity is so apparent in real life 3. different ways to help kids that are labeled 4. lots of interesgrg little facts in Km 1. pros and cons of inclusive education 2. classroom techniques for including diverse students 3. how to celebrate diversity in the classroom Jill I. about ADD and other learning disorders 2. about diversity in general 3. all the commrmitym My 1. important to celebrate diversity in the classroom 2. laws about inclusion and stuff 3. tokeepanopenmind 2. everyone is different and we have to treat people differently . 3. can't label people because ofthe way they look 191 Ian 1. inclusiveness (inclusion) 1. being more sensitive to others' needs 2. discrimination 2. lot of things that she looks at differently 3. communicating with people who are diffaent 3. understanding that in the classroom there are 4. gender issues going to be kids who are different 5. different lifestyles, like gay/lesbian mid 1n 6. labeling 1mg Kae Ilsa 1. mostly about diversity, learning about different 1. remembering that everyone doesn't come from c same background 2. beawareofspecialneedskids 3. importanttocooperatewithpeoplewelearn with 108 . . . I have a lot more experience with multiculturalism and diversity because of the way I was raised with my mom than a lot of people do. It really shocked me how naive some people were as far as the biases and the prejudices that still go on. I've always known that my parents were real big on treating everybody the same, blah, blah, blah, but I never really realized how different that was from how a lot of people are raised. [Tina 422-44] At the end of the course another high student, Cindy, reported that she had become more confident about her decision to work with special education students. . . .back in January, going into special ed. and everything, I wasn't positive that I'd like, you know, special ed. differences like that. I didn't know if I'd be able to handle it. And now I think I know that I can handle it. [Cindy 506-16] For Ernest taking the course led to a greater awareness of his feelings about the issues presented. "It just made me think a little bit about how I really did feel about certain issues” [Ernest 425-12]. Kathy Ieamed about the importance of keeping an open mind, and Judy came to the realization that "I just didn't know as much as I thought I did about the way things are being run in education today" [Judy 425-28]. Although four of the low students reported learning nothing new about themselves from the course, it is important to note that the other three low students did report leamings that seem similar to those reported by the seven high students. For example, Kate reported that she had Ieamed to change her tendency to prejudge people who are different. Not to label and treat people the same but then some people different. I don't know if you understand that, but just not to label anymore. I used to label and [I Ieamed] just not to do that and to treat people like for who they are and not for what they look like and how they act. [Kate 421-12] Bob apparently gained a new awareness of the benefits of interacting with people who have disabilities or special needs. I guess the personal and emotional benefits that can come from interacting with people with disabilities or special needs, you know, like people that are blind or deaf. It broadens your understanding of the human condition, I guess, and your compassion for people and respect for people. [Bob 426-32] While she did not identify specific leamings about herself, Joy seemed very sure that she had Ieamed things that were going to make her a better person and a better teacher. 109 Yeah, just everything that I'm taking away from here that's making me, one, a better person, two, more open to differences and changes, and all that's going to do is help me become, one, a better person, and, two, a better teacher, you know, and a better mom, too, someday. [Joy 428-26] The fact that highs in this study were more likely than lows to report learning about themselves from the course may well be coincidental, but a number of other explanations seem equally possible. For instance, highs may have been inclined to be more self-aware than lows, they may have been better able to see connections between the course content and their personal lives, they may have been more open to the material being presented, or they simply may have found the course content more personally relevant than the lows did. In the final interview students were asked to identify the class sessions which they considered the most valuable and those they considered the least valuable. My analysis of student responses to this question revealed few noteworthy differences between highs and lows. Table 4.10 lists the class sessions which highs and lows identified as most and least valuable. (The numbers in parentheses indicate the number of students who mentioned each class session.) "The F.A.T. City" video, which dealt with strategies for working with students with learning disabilities, was rated by eleven of the fourteen studentsufive highs and six lowsuas one of the most valuable class sessions. No other session was mentioned by more than five students. In identifying the class sessions which they considered most valuable, high students mentioned five sessions which were not cited by the lows, while the lows mentioned three sessions not cited by the highs. There were eight sessions that were named by students in both groups as being valuable. As for the least valuable class sessions, only three sessions were mentioned by students in each group. High students identified two sessions not identified by lows as being the least valuable, while the low students identified three sessions not mentioned by the highs. Perhaps the only noteworthy difference between the two groups of students on this particular question was that more low students identified more sessions that they considered not valuable. Among the lows, six different sessions were identified as least 110 Table 4.10 Most/Least Valuable Class Sessions Hfgh Students Low Students (5) "F.A.T. City” video (6) F.A.T. City video (2) bi/gay/lesbian panel (3) bi/gay/lesbian panel (2) cooperative learning (2) role-playing activities (1) ”Educating Peter" video (1) cooperative learning (1) role-playing activities (3) "Educating Peter” video (l) deaf student panel (3) deaf student panel Most Valuable Sessions ( 1) community panel (2) community panel (1) blind student presentation (1) blind student presentation (2) "True Colors” video (1) Riskbreakers video (1) behavior management (1) "Joey" activity (1) Windwolf article (1) guest speaker on Hawaii (1) trust walk (1) guest speaker on Africa L (2) bilingual education (2) bilingual education (1) cooperative learning (2) cmperative learning Least Valuable Sessions (1) role-playing activities (1) role-playing activities (1) community panel (1) tntst walk (3) last lab session (2) lecture on gender issues (1) bi/gay/lesbian panel valuable a total of eleven times by seven students, whereas among the highs, four different sessions were identified a total of six times by five students. If this difference means anything at all, it may suggest that, overall, lows were either less satisfied with the class sessions or maybe they were just slightly more critical than the highs. These similarities between the high and low students in terms of the class sessions that they identified as most and least valuable suggest to me that the two groups were quite similar in their overall response to the course. They do not support the notion that students with less favorable racial attitudes responded less favorably to the class. Feelings about course. During the final interview I obtained additional evidence on the students' feelings about the course when I asked them whether they felt as though they had gotten more or less than they had expected from the course. Five high students and four low students reported that the course had met or exceeded their expectations (see Table 4.11). Two highs and three lows indicated either that they had had no set 111 Table 4.11 Students' Feelings about the Course High3tudents Low Students Students who had no definite expectafions Cindy Bob but were satisfied with course Claire Kate Students who had dilTerent expectations Kathy but were satisfied with course Students who felt course offered what was Ernest Bill expected Jan Students who felt course offered more than Judy Till what was expected Karen Joy Terri Tina expectations for the course or that they had expected something different; nevertheless, these students all said that they were satisfied with what they had gotten from the course. The fact that four of the high students, as compared to only two of the low students, stated that the course had exceeded their expectations could possibly be interpreted as an indication that high students responded more favorably to the course than did low students; however, I don't believe the data are strong enough to support such an interpretation. While I can say for sure that all fourteen of the target students were satisfied with the course, my data provide me insufficient bases to infer that highs were more satisfied than lows. Students' comments about the course were pretty consistently positive. Asked whether or not she was satisfied with what she had gotten from the course, Claire, a low student, responded, ”Definitely! Most definitely” [Claire 429-56]. Jill, another low student, observed, '1 never got what I had expected. I got more than I expected. I only expected like something and then I got more” [Jill 42680]. A third low student commented, I learned a lot. Actually, I didn't really know exactly what the course was really about going into it. I mean, I just took it pretty much because I needed to take it and I really didn't know what it was about until like the first day and so, yeah, I think I learned a lot. [Kate 421-98] High students were perhaps even more laudatory in their remarks about the class. 112 Do you feel you've got more or less than you expected fiom this class? Oh, a lot more. Easy. I can easily say that. Oh yeah. There was a lot more information given than I ever thought I would ever get in a semester course in college. [Tina 422-86] 1 had the following exchange with another high student. Did you get from this course more or less than you expected to get? More, I think. What makes you say that? 'Cause it made me think about things that I didn't think it would. I kinda thought that I would go in and it would kinda be maybe a rehash of things that I'd heard about in our racial relations [class], but it really made me think about a lot of things and about the way that I treat people and just the way that society and the state and the school system deals with people who are different. [Judy 425-94,96] A third high student offered another revealing remark about the course: Last semester I had 8 o'clocks that I would miss because I'd sleep in or whatever. You know, turn off my alarm or something. This class met two days a week at 8 o'clock and I was there every time, except when I went on vacation one time. But you know, I never even once thought about skipping this class. I don't know if that really tells you anything. [Cindy 506—62] Although some of the data reported earlier suggest that high students and low students may have come away from the course with different things, my data also suggest that there were no practical differences between highs and lows in terms of their satisfaction with the course. Eflect of being interviewed. Because I wondered whether or not the bi-weekly interviews would influence students' perceptions of the course or the course material, during the final interview I asked each student whether they felt that the interviews had influenced their perceptions of the course. Eleven of the fourteen target students (five highs and six lows) said that the interviews had had little or no influence on any of their beliefs. When low students were asked whether talking with me had affected their perceptions of the course, Jill, for example, responded, "I don't think so. Not too much. I mean, if it has, it's been in a very minute way” [Jill 426-220]. Claire said, ”As far as my perceptions of how I looked at an issue, I don't think it changed it” [Claire 429-112]. Kate responded, "No, because you don't really give any opinion on anything" [Kate 421- 230], and Joy answered, “Probably not, 'cause I think people are very set in their ways. I 113 think it made me aware, but I don't think it changed me" [Joy 428-191]. High students offered similar responses to this question. Tina said, ”I think it makes me think more. I don't know if it's changed it so much as it's made me think about it more" [Tina 422-230]. Kathy responded, "It may have. Not anything that I'm conscious of, I don't think” [Kathy 506-160]. Cindy and Judy also indicated that they had been pushed to think more but their actual views had not been changed by the interviews. Karen thought that though she had become less sure of some of her beliefs, her views had not been changed. Would you say that at some point talking with me changed your perceptions of something? Just that you made me a little bit less sure in some things. Okay, but I didn't actually change you, though? Not yet. [Karen 427-121,123] Only three of the target students felt as though any of their beliefs had been substantially affected by the interviews. Bob, one of the low students, seemed unsure about the extent to which he was influenced by the interviews I'm sure it's had some effect on the issues, because you've questioned me on them and I hear racial reactions. You're like, "You're sure you think that?" you know. ”Aren't you to blame for that, too?" Sol think that's had an effect on me, more personally, thinking about it more. And maybe possibly made me a little more biased in one direction. I did? Possibly. Have I changed any of your perceptions? It probably hasn't that much. Possibly in causing me to review and rethink about the different things we did in the class. Made me more aware of it and to think about it a little more, you know, and put it more in my mind. [Bob 426- 253,255,259] Terri, one of the high students, was more sure that her views had been influenced by the interviews. Did talking with me actually change some of your perceptions? In some ways I think it did, and in some ways I think that", I don't know. I think sometimes, like, I couldn't--, there were some things that I really, you know, was like in the middle. I really didn't know what to say here and there, and I think that talking with you did help work out a lot of those things, to help me understand, you know,why I felt that way, but I don't really know. A couple of things were changed. [Ten'i 426-15'7] Ernest, another high student, was also pretty definite that his views had been affected by the interviews. 114 Has talking with me this semester affected your perceptions of the course or the way you think about any of the issues of the course? Yeah, I think so. Can you tell me how? Well just, I mean, I thought a lot more about the class. I think I've got a lot more out of the class because of this. This is the only time I'm involved in any type of discussion about these issues, and that kind of thing kinda makes me think a little bit and then that makes me maybe reconsider or reaf f irm what I feel. Or it just kind of makes me think about the course a little bit more. Okay. You said it makes you think about the ideas more. More in depth. Do you think it's actually changed the way you thought about any of the issues? Yeah. In fact, specifically, I recall talking about whether gay/billesbians should be able to have kids or not. We talked about that and I kinda thought about it, and then I--not to sound wishy washy--but I kinda rethought about it. You know, when it was laid out on the table, yeah, you know. Why not? I guess. Whereas it was just kinda this thing inside me that said, you know, it's kinda wrong. But there's not really good reasons behind it. [Ernest 425-166,168, 170] These three students each credited the interviews with changing their perceptions about something; however, their explanations actually suggest that the interviews merely stimulated them to think more deeply about the course content and about their beliefs and then to change their opinions. Bob said that the interviews led him to "review and rethink about the different things we did in the class,” Teni asserted that "talking with you did help work out a lot of those things, to help me understand, you know, why I felt that way," and Ernest felt pushed to "think a little bit and then that makes me maybe reconsider or reaffirm what I feel." Since the fourteen target students were nearly unanimous-Jill, a low student, was the only exception-~in saying that they believed that the interviews had pushed them to think more deeply about the course material and about their own beliefs, I don't believe that the interviews necessarily affected Bob, Teni, or Ernest any more than they did the other target students; rather, I believe that these three students were simply more aware of particular ways in which their views had changed through the self examination and rethinking that the interviews had stimulated. During my bi-weekly interviews, with the target students, I made a conscious effort to question many of the beliefs that the students expressed, continually pushing them to explain and justify themselves. Although the interviews were not intended to be an intervention, because they did stimulate more self -examination and more reflection 115 about the course content, I believe that the target students may have gained more from the course than most of the students who weren't interviewed. Ernest (high), for instance, directly stated, "I think I've got a lot more out of the class because of this" [Ernest 425- 168], and Bob (low) indicated that the interviews "made me more aware of [the content] and to think about it a little more, you know, and put it more in my mind” [Bob 426-259]. Most of the other target students also made comments suggesting that their involvement in the interviews had been beneficial. Jan, one of the low students, remarked, Well, it probably helped in thinking about [issues] more, 'cause I've had to think about answers I've given or why I think that way, so yeah, and instead of me just taking it in and maybe not knowing what they were saying, just going home and coming back next week to the lecture, now I'm forced to think about it. [Jan 429-] One of the high students made a very similar comment. It's helped me to think a lot of my thoughts on things, sol think that does help. You don't just listen to a lecture and then, you know, take it in and then not really ever think. Some people, I'm sure, don't ever think about it again. There are some parts of this class that I've done that, you know. You take it in, but that's it, you know. So this has helped me to rethink things and not necessarily rethink them but like just to go over them again. [Cindy 506-178] Joy, a low student, commented a couple of different times on the value of being interviewed. Early in the final interview she stated ”...just through talking to you because you made me, like, realize a lot of things about myself that I probably wouldn't have realized unless I would have talked to you...." [Joy 4284]. Later in the same interview she said, "I think it's made me more aware about my views on different races and different subject areas. You've made me think a lot about myself and just what I thought" [Joy 428-189]. Kate, another low student, also reported that the interviews had been helpful to her. Did the interviews have any impact on how you see the course or how you think about the issues? Yeah, in some ways. How so? Just 'cause if I say an answer, you kind of come back and say something else or ask me why, and then I've got to explain it, and it just kind of helps me get in my head or whatever, like the lesbian/bi/ gay, like that whole subject we talked about. That kind of changed my thinking. [Kate 421-234,236] 116 Several of the high students also offered insightful comments about ways in which they found the interviews helpful. Kathy observed that ”it probably makes me think about the issues more than I would have. Definitely" [Kathy 506-162]. Judy seemed to find value in having her ideas questioned: "You just brought up different kinds of points and stuff like that, just different points of view and kind of made me think a little bit, kinda challenged what I was saying, you know. You challenged my ideas mostly" [Judy 425- 270]. Karen found that having her ideas questioned in the interviews caused her to examine her beliefs more closely. So I think you're telling me that when I question you about your beliefs, it makes you think more deeply about them. Is that what you're saying? Yeah. Made me think more deeply about it. Why I believe it's right and for what reasons and where that comes from and what do I have to back it up. [Karen 427- 1 19] The interviews pushed students to think more deeply about their beliefs and as a result of this extra thinking, some of them modified some of their beliefs. For example, Terri told me that her feelings about the acceptability of Black English had changed as a result of discussing the issue with me. Was that change because of the class or because of talking to me? I think talking with you 'cause you kept asking me more questions and asking me this and, "Well, you're saying this, but you're saying that," you know, just kind of like I was contradicting myself a lot and a lot of different things, and I think that when I got back in my room whatever and I thought about it and I realized how stupid I sounded and I, like, thought about it, you know, all the questions you asked me and stuff, yeah, I do think that most of it did come from talking to you. But it came from your thinking about [ inaudible, overlapping talk] Yeah, my thinking about it [inaudible overlapping talk] «not my saying you should think this way or--- No, you never said, "You should." You just kept asking me and saying, ”You feel like this," and with you saying that, you know, it made it like sound [inaudible] that's how I think whatever, and it bothered me. [Teni 426-159,161,163] Finally, Tina explained how she thought the interviews had pushed her to become more engaged with the course material. Has talking with me this semester afiected either your perceptions of the course or the way you think about any of the issues covered in the course? I think it makes me think more. I don't know if it's changed it so much as it's made me think about it more. How so? 117 (Pause) Because I knew I was gonna have to talk to you about it, about whatever. I had to think about it sol could form my own opinion and make rational thought, most of the time. And in that sense, I don't think it changed what I learned, as far as it made me explore it more. Sol guess--, I can't spit out what I'm trying to say. I think it affects not what I thought, but how I thought it. Which doesn't make any sense whatsoever. (Pause) I think regardless of if I were to talk to you or not, I would have come out thinking the same way I think. Okay. But because I've had to talk to you, I think it deeper than I would have thought, not talking to you. So thinking more deeply about things but not changing anything? Right. But it's made me think about it maybe more. Whereas before I would have said, "Oh, yep. Good point [and then] store it away for future knowledge.” This way I said, "Why is it a good point, you know? How is it gonna affect the way I work? Da dada da.” That applied it. There you go! That's the word! Talking to you has made me apply what I've leamed!--instead of just storing it away in some little back comer of my head, going "Oh yup. Interesting. Wow.” [Tina 422-230,232,234,238] Thus, though the interviews were not intended to be an intervention, all of the preceding excerpts strongly suggest that they had considerable effect on the target students' thinking about the course material. The excerpts further suggest that these students may have been more deeply affected by their experience in the course than were others in the class who were not interviewed and who may not have been pushed to think so deeply. It is worth noting, however, that high students and low students seemed to have reacted similarly to the interviews. Response to Black interviewer. All fourteen of my target students were White, and I wondered whether and to what extent their responses to the interview questions may have been influenced by my being a Black person. Therefore, I asked each student this question in the final interview. Low and high students alike told me that my race had had little or no influence on how they had responded to my questions. For instance, I had the following exchange with one of the low students: Did it ever make any dtfierence to you that you were being interviewed by a Black person? Uh uh. Even when we discussed racial issues? No, not really. So you 've been honest in expressing your opinions. ? Yeah. Yeah. [Bill 428-200,202,208] 118 Bob, another of the low students, answered, ”Perhaps on some racial questions, [but] I don't think that it has too much" [Bob 426-267]. Two of the high students stated more clearly why they felt my race had not influenced their responses. Did it ever make any difi’erence to you that you 're being interviewed by a Black person? No, it didn't make a difference to me. You're Art and I figure you're here interviewing me cause you wanta hear my opinions and you don't wanta hear what I think you think you do. You don't want me to lie about it. I mean, this is something you're writing a paper about and I think you need me to be as honest as possible, and I tried to be. [Karen 427-129] The second high student responded, No. Cause I figured you'd be just as impartial as I am. I mean if I make racial slurs, I think maybe you'd take offense to it, but given my attitudes, I wouldn't think that there should be any cause for tension or anything when discussing racial issues. [Ernest 425-188] Another of the high students reported no awareness of being influenced by my race. I don't think so. It might of, but not that I'm aware of .o I mean, it might of in some subconscious choice of words or whatever but not any conscious effort on my part that changed it. [Tina 422-244] Unlike these five students, however, the majority of the participants admitted that on some occasions they had felt some uneasiness due to our racial difference, especially during our early interviews, as the following dialogue with one of the low students illustrates. Did it make any dtflerence to you that you were being interviewed by a Black person? No. Why wouldn't it afiect what you think or say? I don't know. In some ways it does, but then in some ways it doesn’t. In what ways does it? Does it? 'Cause I'd feel bad talking about some things, but then other things I don't really care. I might hold back a little more or something in saying something. So you hold back; even now you're holding back? No, just like at first. [Kate 421-238,246,248,252] Some of the students had felt uncomfortable because they didn't want to offend me, but they claimed that they had still been honest in answering my questions. Joy, for example, another of the low students, made it clear that her discomfort and her concern about possibly offending me did not stop her from being honest with me. 119 It never mattered to me. I think sometimes when we were talking and stuff, I'd get uncomfortable, like when [talking] about, like, the Black girls in my hall whatever. I didn't want you to take offense, you know, because you were Black whatever, but it wasn't like a big deal to me, at all. But then what's this concern about me being offended by what you said? It was concern of me offending you. That's all. So you 've been honestly telling me about how you feel about things, even the racial things ? ' Um hmm. I have. [Joy 428-195,197,201] A few of the students reported some initial uneasiness because they had felt unsure about how to address me. Terri's comments illustrated this dilemma. Did it make any diflerence to you that you were being interviewed by a Black person? I think at first, because I think today, like, there's different--, somebody wants to be called African American or Black or whatever, and I think sometimes it's kind of hard for me because I mean there are a lot of people that, you know, "I'm American," they'll say, and they don't want to be called African American or whatever, and so it was kind of hard because I didn't know, like, what to say to you when we were talking about a lot of those topics whatever, and I would catch myself saying ”African American” or I would catch myself saying ”Black" and I didn't know if it would offend you whatever way I said it, so I think that was really like the only thing. I mean, nothing personal or anything. [Terri 422-165] I also asked the students if they had ever felt some concern about possibly being viewed as a bigot because of something that they said to me. Most of the students acknowledged having felt this concern on occasion, but they generally asserted, particularly the high students, that it had not deterred them from saying what they thought. Tina made this latter point very clear in her response to my question. So you never thought about the fact that you might say something either that would hurt my feelings or that I might take exception to what you're saying? Yeah, but it still didn't change the fact that I said it. [Tina 422-246] Cindy, another high student, offered a similar response. Were you ever concerned that you might say something which in my mind would make you a bigot? I have been concerned about that, but then again, they are my opinions. They're one person's opinion. And like, I don't know, a few times whatever, like you had been really argumentative and I thought later, like, Man, I wonder if I offended him? But they're my opinions, you know. That's a part of me. I'm sorry if it has offended you. Yeah, I don't want to offend anybody but obviously if I do, I do. Would you say that you 've been honest with me through it all? Oh yeah, I've been honest with you. Most definitely. [Cindy 506-197,199] 120 A couple of the low students made statements suggesting that their concern about being perceived as a bigot sometimes led them to think more carefully about what they said. For instance, Claire explained, Any apprehension about saying something that I would interpret as your being a bigot. Did that ever cross your mind? Well, a couple of times I think it might have. As far as specific examples, I don’t know. . . . There were times where I would think of something to say, and then I’d think, well, is that prejudiced, you know? I don’t know, I guess maybe I just got to questioning myself because, you know, we’ve just been discussing it. . . . [Claire 429-130] Bob also recognized that concerns about being perceived as a bigot might have subtly influenced his responses or those of other students. I would assume sociologically tlrere'd be some subconscious pull to be less, you know, to say yes, I am less prejudiced against Blacks because you're Black or sometimes a hesitation to be as open about racism or whatever. I think especially for a student that is more racist. Concerned about hurting my feelings or being perceived as a bigot or something? Yeah. [Bob 426-269,27 1] I don't believe the fact that these low students admitted that they thought more about some of their responses out of fear of being considered a bigot is necessarily an indication that their responses were any less honest--at least not consciously so--than those of the high students. However, their comments do seem to indicate that most of the lows were more concerned about my perceiving them as being prejudiced than were any of the high students. As illustrated in some of the preceding examples and in Judy's comments below, the high students appeared to be more secure in their own beliefs about not being prejudiced. Was there any concern about hurting my feelings? No, not really. I think you're a pretty tough guy. You can take it. Any concern about me thinking you were a bigot or something for something that you said? No, I don't think I'm a bigot, so-- [Judy 425286288290] Perhaps because the high students knew themselves that they were not bi gots, whether or not I thought they were was less important to them, and they were more willing to speak their minds, regardless of what I might think of them. 121 In conclusion, while some of the fourteen target students were more conscious of our racial difference than others, it appears that none of them believed that my race had any significant bearing on how they had responded to the interview questions. Consequently, I am inclined to also believe that this variable did not have a major influence on students' responses to the interview questions, especially not the responses of the high students. Nevertheless, I acknowledge that these students' perceptions of their response to a Black interviewer may not have been accurate and that it is still possible that many of the students, both high and low, were affected in ways of which they were unaware. A Close-up Look at Three Students Up until this point, I have been examining only groups of students and reporting similarities and differences between these groups. Now I would like to take a close-up look at three individuals from my group of target students to identify their noteworthy characteristics and to examine how they each responded to the course. I selected three students whom I felt would be illustrative of the range of students in the target group. The first student, Claire, resembled four other low-scoring students in terms of the beliefs and attitudes she expressed and in the ways that she responded to the course material and activities. The second student, Joy, was very similar to one other low student in that, during the course of the semester, both of them seemed to be critically re-evaluating their present beliefs and were moving toward developing more favorable racial attitudes. Although Terri, the third student, had considerably more prior experience with diversity than the six other hi gh-scoring target students, she was nonetheless quite similar to them in terms of the beliefs and attitudes she expressed in our interviews and in the ways that she responded to the course material and activities. In this section I will begin by providing some background information on each of these three students. Then I will discuss how these students seemed to respond differently to some of the course content Finally, I will discuss how some of their personal qualities seemed to influence how they responded to the course. 122 Student Profiles Claire (low) Claire, a sophomore, is the youngest child in a middle-class family of four. She attended high school in a small suburban town where the minority student population was extremely small. Claire's high school activities were limited to band, track, and Students Against Drunk Driving, and she also held an after-school job. Claire reported some personal involvement in a nondenominational church during high school, though it was apparently not sustained, and she gave no indication of the extent that her parents were involved. Her father has an associate's degree and is employed as a firefighter in the neighboring city. Her mother works in a university dormitory and just recently earned her degree from a local business college. Although it is difficult to say much about her relationship with her parents, some of her statements during the interviews indicate that they have had a strong impact on her thinking in several areas. Claire described herself as a very outgoing person who is not a follower. Although she had made friends with people of different races since coming to the university, all of her close friends were White because, as she explained it, those were the people that she had grown up with; it was not because she didn't like other people. At the beginning of this course, Claire's awareness of prejudice and discrimination seemed to be quite limited, and she did not appear to be particularly open to evidence of their continuing existence. Her own experience seemed to be the standard by which she judged the veracity of allegations of discrimination. For example, because of her own experiences in school, she rejected the notion that women and minorities face unequal opportunities to learn. I don't believe that these people that are listed-the minorities, special needs, gids, etc.-face unequal opportunities. I'm a girl. My major is math, sol kind of like prove it wrong in a sense there. If somebody has a will, then they'll go as far as they want to, and whether a person does go far, it's not because they don't have the opportunity. I just believe it's because they don't want to. [Claire 211-70] She didn't seem to even consider the possibility that the schooling experiences of others might have been vastly differently than hers. Throughout the semester Claire professed 123 an exceptionally strong belief that everyone has equal opportunities, and as a corollary, she also believed that a person's success or failure was dependent strictly on his/her own efforts. I was always told when I was young, you know, I can do whatever I want, and I think it's that way with anybody. I don't see why it would make a difference if you're minority or special need or anything. If that's what you want to do, you're going to set your goal and work towards it. [Claire 211-74] Although she considered herself to be unprejudiced and open-minded, Claire seemed to possess some stereotypical beliefs about racial minorities. First, she seemed to conceive of them as being poor, and she acknowledged that sometimes lack of money could constrain their opportunities. She also seemed to believe that the lower school achievement of minority children is a consequence of their problem-filled home environments. Third, when asked for an additional explanation of why most minority students don't perform as well in school as most White students, Claire responded, I don't know. Maybe it's hereditary and the genes don't carry through to carry a lot of information. I mean, I don't think it's because they've been taught less or something to that effect, 'cause like I said before, everybody's got the same opportunity, I believe. [Claire 211-84] In her perception, since all students have an equal opportunity to learn, if minority students are not achieving on a par with White students, it must be the result of some deficiency in the minorities, perhaps a hereditary deficiency. Claire discounts the possibility that schools or teachers mi ght contribute to the achievement discrepancy. Joy (low). Joy comes from a middle- to upper middle-class family of five, the oldest of three girls. From a large Detroit suburb, she attended a high school which she described as ”very diverse,“ though, based on her descriptions, it sounded as though the largest minority group was the Caldearrs. Joy was very active in high school activities, including student government and sports. During the last three years of high school, she coached elementary and junior high girls' basketball teams. There was never any mention of her or her family being involved with a church. Both of her parents are college graduates. Joy's mother teaches in a nearby suburban city and her father owns a 124 manufacturing business. Joy did not talk much about her father, but she seemed to be quite proud of her mother. It also appears that her mother had a major impact on Joy's life, particulariy on some of her attitudes toward minorities and her beliefs about teaching. Joy reported that her circle of friends, close and casual, was not very diverse, instead being very much like her. She seemed to be a bright, bubbly, friendly person. She was usually quite talkative and always seemed to be open and honest. Joy took her involvement in the research quite seriously and was the only one of the participants who regularly came to the interviews with a few notes written down about things she wanted to say. At the beginning of the course Joy seemed to be somewhat more aware of prejudice and discrimination than was Claire. For example, regarding new immigrants to this country, she said, "I don't feel that we should be able to, like, discriminate because a child doesn't really understand English that well.” It is noteworthy that she recognized the failure to provide equal learning opportunities for immigrants as a type of discrimination. Joy also expressed the belief that being open-minded and accepting people's differences were important. You have to realize that everybody's different and because they believe in something different than you doesn't make them wrong; it doesn't make them horrible people whatever; that's just their views. What I think we need to teach our children is that being different is okay. [Joy 128-34] On the question of whether schools, along with celebrating Christmas, should also celebrate the holidays of other religions, Joy commented that "Sometimes the majority shouldn't always rule. I think we need to accept everyone's views." Further evidence of Joy's belief in being accepting of differences comes from her response to a question about how she, as a teacher, would respond to a group of shy Native American students who were not participating in class. I'm going to want a lot of participation because I think people learn through participation, being in active discussion and stuff, but I would never want to, like, make the children feel uncomfortable if that was their background, that maybe they were shy. . . . I mean, if that's what they have learned and that's what their customs are. [Joy 128-99] 125 The fact that this belief appeared in different forms at different points in the interview suggests that it was a sincere and deeply held one. Finally, Joy acknowledged that minority students do not have equal opportunities to learn in school, and she posited the major reason as being the unconscious prejudice of teachers. Despite her open-mindedness, her acceptance of differences, and her awareness of prejudice and discrimination, Joy also held some other beliefs that would appear to be problematic in working with diverse learners. First, a recurrent theme in her first interview was the belief that working hard pays off, a belief that was instilled in her by her mother. I mean, [my mother] taught me from when I was a little girl, you know. We'd go out and play basketball together. She taught me everything, and she always taught me that working hard pays off. [Joy 128-24] As was the case with Claire, this belief led Joy to conclude that when people didn't succeed, it was due to their not being willing to work hard. ' I hope I'm not generalizing, but like Detroit, and how kids aren't seeing that if they work hard in school that that can get 'em just as far as going out and selling drugs. I think a lot of people, like in general, don't understand that working hard pays off. [Joy 128-22] In addition, I think this belief made it easy for Joy to adopt her mother's belief that minority students do poorly in school because they lack motivation and don't come from good homes. Her mother teaches in a school with a large percentage of minority children, and Joy commented, She is constantly thinking of motivating, because that's what these children», they just need motivating right now. I mean, they're just so stuck in a rut, and a lot of these children don't come from very good homes. [Joy 128-14] Along a similar vein, Joy believes that one reason White children do better in school is that they come from homes where the expectations are higher. Thus, although Joy seemed to have a genuine belief in the importance of accepting differences, some of her other strongly held beliefs seemed to be in conflict with that belief. Additionally, while Joy said that her high school was "very diverse,” it seems significant that she reported 126 having no Level Three (close) or Level Two (casual) friendships with individuals who were not White. Terri (high). Terri, a freshman, comes from a middle-class family of five, the oldest of three children. She attended a racially diverse school in medium-sized city, where she was very active in a wide variety of school activities. Terri and her family have also been active in their church, and her religion seems to be very important to her. She reported having a lot of experience with people who are different than she. Terri appeared to be quite sensitive to and compassionate toward others. Her high level of involvement in school and church activities, as well as numerous comments that she made during the initial interview, suggests that she is a doer; she wants to be the kind of person who goes out and changes things. She wants to make a difference. Terri has a boyfriend whom she described as biracial, and she said that at first her parents had a lot of trouble accepting that finally, Terri’s reports of past incidents in her life seem to suggest that she is a strong and a principled person who will stand up for what she believes in. In numerous ways Teni demonstrated greater awareness and a much deeper understanding of the problem of prejudice and discrimination than did either Claire or Joy. In our initial interview she commented that because of her background, she had found during the first few class sessions that she was farther along the path of accepting and celebrating diversity than were many of the other students in the class. My friends are from all different backgrounds, all different races, and when I sit in the class and when we talk in our lab, I hear people saying, 'I've never met anyone that's different than me. My school was all White. My school was all this." Well, my school was very diverse. I learned all that. I mean, I just think that a lot of the experiences I've had and knowing what to do and what not to do, I’ve learned definitely not to stereotype people, and I see so many people doing it here. It's because they don't know at all, and I think that I've been educated well enough. I mean, I just think that I have had enough experiences right now that a lot of these things we read about, a lot of these things I have already known, I've already experienced, but there's a lot of people in the class that are looking at this for the first time, and they're just thinking, "Oh, my gol.” It's so new to them, but to me it's not new at all. [Terri 131-60] 127 In many respects, Terri began the semester already possessing much of the awareness we hoped to develop in students taking the course. Although she believed that minorities had equal opportunities to learn at her high school, she demonstrated a fairly good understanding of how learning opportunities were not equal elsewhere. Terri believed that the lower school achievement by minority students was due to several factors: lower expectations on the part of the teachers, poor student motivation, and lack of appr0priate parental influence. In talking about the lower school achievement of minority students and about the Native Americans in the vignette, Terri clearly demonstrated a reluctance to make broad generalizations about racial/cultural groups. In addition to her boyfriend, who is biracial, Terri indicated that several of her closest friends were Black. As can be seen from the background descriptions, each of the three students began the course with different beliefs about racial minorities and different levels of awareness of racism and discrimination. My analysis of our interviews across the semester revealed that these three students also appeared to have reacted differently to some of the information and activities and to have Ieamed different things from the course. While I believe that these outcome differences are primarily attributable to the differences in students' beliefs and attitudes that existed at the outset of the course, I also believe that students’ personal qualities played an important role. In this section I will explain how the students reacted differently to course content, what they learned from the course, and how some of their personal qualities seemed to influence their responses to the course content. I_-Iow Students Reacfl to Course Content Although all three students were exposed to the same information and to the same activities during the CEP 240 course, their responses to the material were often quite different. In fact, it appeared as though the students' racial attitudes and beliefs served as a filter through which they interpreted the information and the experiences that they were presented with in the course. In other words, it appeared as though the meaning that the 128 students gave to a particular lesson or activity was at least partly a function of the attitudes and beliefs through which they perceived the lesson or activity. In this section of the paper I will highlight two particular course activities and illustrate how these three students' responded differently to the activities. The first class activity that provides a good illustration of the differences in the students' responses was a videotape of a 20/20 (ABC) television segment entitled "True Colors," which all students viewed in their lab sections early in the semester. In this particular segment two men--one Black and one White-who were very similar in terms of their age, education, socioeconomic status, etc., were asked to go to St. Louis, where they were followed with a hidden camera and microphone as they went shopping, looked for an apartment, applied for jobs, and were placed in various other situations. The two men were treated very differently in nearly every situation, and the major message of the video was that racial discrimination is still a problem in our society. Claire, Joy, and Terri each responded differently to their viewing of this video. Claire. In mid-February during her second interview with me, I asked Claire for her reaction to the ”True Colors“ video. I really didn't think there was that much difference out in the real world. I kind of sat there in shock. . . . I guess I just didn't realize that it was that different, you know, that they're treated that different [Claire 211-367] So, her initial reaction was one of great surprise. She said that she did not realize that Blacks were treated so differently. I said to her, 'But you saw evidence, didn't you?” and Claire responded, ”Yeah, this is true. Unfortunately, it obviously happens" [Claire 211- 388]. A short time later in the same interview, after some additional discussion, I asked Claire, "The people who treated those two guys differently, what would you call what they were doing?" Claire's response was, "Definitely discrimination” [Claire 211-438]. I asked this question to ascertain whether or not she recognized that the video was illustrating the existence of racial discrimination. Apparently, she did recognize it. 129 Two months later in mid-April during our fifth interview, I had the following exchange (edited) with Claire: In what ways is it a benefit to be White in this country? I don't really see any. I guess in society you make it out to be that there are advantages to being White. I don't see 'em that way. [Claire 412-225] Claire's comment seems to be inconsistent with the message of the "True Colors” video, which showed that the White man had the advantage of not being discriminated against. Her comment is consistent, however, with the belief that she expressed numerous times in the first interview: that everyone has equal opportunities. If Claire were to admit that there are advantages to being White, it would be difficult for her to also believe that everyone has equal opportunities. After some more discussion, I showed Claire a newspaper article with statistics on the income differential between Whites and minorities in the metropolitan area I then asked her to explain the discrepancy in income. I don't think it's because there isn't an equal chance. I think everybody has an equal opportunity, and especially now and within the last couple years because they've become more aware of not hiring somebody because they're Black or whatever. [Claire 412-298] Even in the face of contradictory evidence, Claire clings to her belief that everyone has an equal opportunity. She doesn't know why there is such a wide discrepancy in the income levels of Whites and minorities, but she refuses to even entertain the notion that it could be because of differential treatment. As our discussion continued, Claire made a comment suggesting that a person's income level is related to their educational level. I asked her whether Whites tend to have more education than Blacks and Hispanics. No, I don't think so at all. Like I said, everybody has an equal opportunity as far as who wants to pursue it more; that's their option. This article shows unemployment in the metropolitan area. Do you have any theories as to why there are more Blacks and Hispanics unemployed than there are Whites? I think if I had seen that for, like, ten years ago, I would have said it was because of the problem with not employing somebody because they are Black or because they are Hispanic. As far as now, I have no idea. 130 . . .As far as why there's more Blacks and Hispanics unemployed, I don't know, but there's a variety of reasons for unemployment, and it's not because of race or color. [Claire 412-298] Claire continues to cling tenaciously to her belief that everyone has equal opportunities, despite being presented with more information to the contrary. She doesn't know why the unemployment discrepancy exists, but once again she refuses to even allow the possibility that it could be because of racial discrimination. In mid-February, after watching the "True Colors” video, Claire appeared to recognized that racial discrimination still occurs, but apparently during the next two months, she either had forgotten or had reconstructed the message of that video. In our final interview, when I asked Claire to identify for me the three most important things that she Ieamed from the course, she said, "I know one of the things that I noticed was that diversity is so apparent in real life, butl guess I never realized it, like the ‘True Colors,‘ when we watched that. It was really interesting" [Claire 429-2]. A short time later, when I asked her to identify the class sessions that she thought were the most valuable, Claire responded, ”I really liked ‘True Colors.’ I found that really interesting of how the diversity is so different and so intense out there" [Claire 429-46]. Still later in the interview, as she is responding to the question ”Why do most poor and minority students not perform as well in school?” Claire makes the following comment: ”If you relate it to the movie ‘True Colors,’ like, you know, the Black guy went to get a job and he couldn't get 'em, you know; it was the White population out there" [Claire 429-64]. The fact that Claire refers to the ”True Colors” three times in this interview is irrefutable evidence that she had not forgotten about it. Not only has she not forgotten about it, she identifies it as one of the most valuable parts of the class and its message as one of her most valuable leamings. The problem, however, is that she has recast the message. In her mind, the video was about diversity, not about discrimination. Although it is not clear what she meant by ”diversity,“ it is clear from her earlier comments that she knows what discrimination is, and she chooses not to use the term ”discrimination" in 131 reference to the video. Therefore, the meaning that she has given to the video is different than what was intended. But by giving the video a different meaning, Claire was able to hold on to her belief that everyone has equal opportunities, as the following passage from later in the same interview illustrates. Do you think minorities have equal opportunities to learn? Yeah. Again, there's always going to be people out there that are prejudiced, but I think they have the same equal opportunities. Okay, you 're saying minorities have the same opportunities. What are you basing that on? Especially, again, like lately, like they can't ask you what your race is on a job application, and I think it's just progressing further that they have just as much opportunity as everyone else. [Claire 42982.84] It seems clear that the original impact and message of ”True Colors” has been lost. Its message of discrimination was not consistent with Claire's belief that everyone has equal opportunities, and because she could not simultaneously acknowledge that discrimination exists and at the same time hold on to her belief that everyone has equal opportunities, she reconstructed the meaning of the video so that it would not conflict with her belief, rather than change her belief. Joy. The ”True Colors” video had quite a different impact on Joy than it did on Claire. In our second interview Joy offered her reactions to viewing the video. I watched that in my sociology class last semester. To me it's just fascinating how in this day and age how everything's supposed to be so equal and equality blah, blah, blah, how things are not equal. I think it's not even just on the Black-White issue, 'cause I know I've been in stores, like we talked about this in our recitation. I've been in stores, like, by myself and because I'm a teenager, you know, they watch you. They look at you differently, whereas I go in the same store, you know, and I have my mom with me, and they're there to help you, no problem, so I think that it's on all levels, but I think it's really interesting. Because I'd seen it before, it wasn't as powerful. Do you know what I'm saying? It was more powerful the first time I saw it, but it was really neat to, like, see people in my class going, ”I can't believe this really happens,” you know. It was really, really fascinating to me. [Joy 210-228] Because she had seen the video the previous semester, Joy did not react to it with the same surprise as did Claire. Joy does indicate, however, that the first time she saw it, it had had a powerful impact on her. Her observation that "things are not equal” seems to be an indication that she was clearly aware that the message of the video was that racial 132 discrimination is still a problem in this society. Additionally, her comment that it is not just a Black-White issue and her relating of her own experience with being discriminated against in stores suggest that she began the course with a greater awareness of discrimination than did Claire. For Joy, therefore, viewing the video again served as a reaffirrnation of something she already knew, and she seemed to derive satisfaction from watching many of her classmates' reactions of surprise to the events in the video. Two months later, I asked Joy the same question I asked Claire: "Do you believe that there are benefits to being White in America?” Joy responded as follows, Definitely. It goes back to in lecture how we saw the Black man and the White man who were totally equal and the White man was getting so many more opportunities and not, like in the store, not looked at as maybe a thief or whatever, and as a White person, with my parents being, like, middle-class whatever, I feel I got a lot of--, a very good education, like before college, and then being White also. It goes back to money. Being able to come to Michigan State and get an even better education. So definitely I think being White has advantages. [Joy 407-449] - Not only does she readily admit that there are benefits to being White, she also cites the "True Colors” video as evidence for her belief. In doing so, she appears to demonstrate an understanding that discrimination against one group generally translates into or is associated with advantages for another group. In contrast to Claire, who apparently forgot about the message of ”True Colors" two months later when discussing the condition of minorities in this society, Joy did not forget and seemed to use it as one basis for her belief that Whites are advantaged in this society. Because she had already acknowledged that racial discrimination exists in this society and that Whites have numerous advantages as a result of this discrimination, I did not ask Joy to explain the discrepancies between the education and income of Whites and minorities, as I did with Claire. It was pretty evident to me that Joy already possessed this awareness. In my final interview with Joy, she made two additional references to the "True Colors” video which clearly indicated that, in contrast to Claire, she had neither forgotten nor reconstructed its central message. First, she identified the "True Colors” video as one 133 of the class sessions that had been most valuable for her. She offered the following explanation. ”True Colors” was just the fact that--, it was a realization for me of how prejudiced our society is, just in the simple fact of you have the Black man and the White man going to the store. I mean, (sound effect), it just (sound effect). I don't know; it bothered me. Just the outright prejudice, you know, that you could see on the camera, and then when people got caught, [they'd say] "I didn't do it" I mean, what are you talking about? We were watching you do it right now, you know. [Joy 428-60] Clearly, Joy was able to recall the central message that the video had presented. Her second reference to the video actually came at an earlier point in the final interview, when I had asked her to identify the three most important things she had Ieamed from the class. As one of her leamings Joy mentioned discrimination. Our dialogue then proceeded as follows: What do you mean by you learned about discrimination? Give me an example or something. Okay, when we watched the movie on-- "True Colors "? Yeah, "True Colors” whatever. It's kind of like I always knew it was out there, but literally seeing it in front of my face, I realized, God, I mean, like the store owner automatically suspected that the Black person was going to steal something, and I think there's been times where I thought that also, like working in the store, and I realize that I must have a little bit of discrimination also in myself, and I realize that that's wrong, like I shouldn't have those biases and those prejudices against-u Why is that an important learning to you? Because I think it's going to help me out. If I realize I am now, in the classroom hopefully I'll be able to keep everyone equal, you know, instead of being like-. I'm learning all about separate but equal, you know. I don't want that. Equal is equal, you know. Whether you're Black or whether you're White or whether you're Hispanic or Asian, I think all children should be regarded the same, and that's my responsibility, to make sure that my views, maybe stuff that, like I said before, has been instilled in me from a long time ago, that that doesn't come out, because as a teacher I'm a huge role model for these children. I'm a mom to 30 children, and if they see my prejudice and negative views, that's what they're going to take from me, and I have to set an example for these kids. [Joy 428-6,8,10] While at the end of the course Claire did not even acknowledge the existence of racial discrimination in our society, Joy definitely does. In fact, Joy's remarks show that she has moved beyond simply having an increased awareness of it in the larger society; she also reports realizing that she herself has discriminated. She seems to have internalized 134 her awareness and is now able to see biases not only in others but also in herself. For her, racial discrimination is no longer just something "out there”; she recognizes that she herself has biases and that, as a future teacher, she needs to overcome those biases for the benefit of her students. Certainly, Joy, much moreso than Claire, shows evidence that she is moving toward developing the awareness and attitudes that will enable her to work more effectively with minority children. Terri. Because Terri began the course with a pretty well developed awareness of racism and discrimination, her response to the ”True Colors” video was quite different from that of Claire or Joy. Having grown up in a diverse community and having attended a high school that was approximately 50% minority (predominantly African American), Terri brought to the course considerable knowledge of and experience with diversity. Early in the semester Terri became aware of just how different her background was from that of most of her classmates and how much more aware of racial issues she was. In her first interview with me Terri commented, I think that based on a lot of the experiences that I've experienced in [my hometown] that it makes me a stronger person. A lot of pe0ple who come here and see things firsthand are really surprised in the way some people act and some ways people react and that. I feel that I've already experienced a lot of that, considering a lot of the things that have happened to me, and so while everybody else is reacting to what they've seen, I can just look at it from a different perspective because I've already seen a lot of the things that people from little towns haven't seen at all and only see on TV. I know the difference between stereotypical things or things that really happen because you learn to decipher between those things. [Terri 131-16] Although she doesn't explicitly mention racism or discrimination in the preceding passage, I believe that that is what she was making reference to because most of the eariy class sessions addressed those two topics. As I had done with Claire and Joy, during my second interview with Terri I asked for her reaction to the ”True Colors” video. . . . I've already seen it before. I believe my reaction was pretty much the same. I think that it does happen. . . . I think that if everyone could watch that tape and people [could] see that it happens. You know, there's too many people in the United States that say, "Racism doesn't happen. This and this doesn't happen.” You can say all those stories, but it's never happened to you. People just assume that people aren't going to be that rude or aren't going to—, you know, that they aren't consciously doing it when maybe they are consciously doing it. They are, 135 and it really can hurt people, so I do think that that was very eye-opening for people who haven't seen it yet. It opened my eyes, definitely, when I watched it last semester. So if I asked you what you learned fiom it, you 'd say what? Well, that racism does exist and that people do treat others differently, I would have to say. Even though you went to a diverse high school, you weren't aware of that? Oh, yeah. I mean, I have a boyfriend who is [biracial]. I mean, we have gotten stares; we've gotten this and that I mean, I've heard comments. Standing in line at Cedar Point, some guy said, "What a waste, and blah blah blah.” I mean, I hear comments all the time. I got comments from my parents. I got comments from whomever, sol mean, I myself have seen it and have felt it--not to an extreme of, you know, a lot of people who go through it everyday, but I mean, I at least know that it exists. Definitely. So then how did this video open your eyes if you were already---? I just think that a lot of the different varieties that it came in--at the store and this and that, so I can see a store and I can understand. I've seen that before, so that wasn't really anything new, but even the car salesman, the man himself who was Black and didn't rush out there either, and the different prices [that they were quoted] and when [the salespeople] were confronted about it. It's just a lot of different things. I mean, I've never gone and priced a car, so I've never seen that before, so it's really just a lot of the different areas that you found it in. [Terri 217-312330334342] Like Joy, Terri had seen the ”True Colors" video in a class the previous semester, but whereas for Joy the initial viewing of the video represented a surprising introduction to the reality of racial discrimination, for Terri viewing the video served primarily to reinforce and extend what she already knew from personal experience. Unlike Joy and many of her classmates, Terri already knew that racism and discrimination existed and that some people truly are as rude and discriminatory as the individuals depicted in the video. As a partner in an interracial relationship, Teni had experienced numerous racial comments directed at her personally, and evidently, she had also witnessed acts of racial discrimination in some settings. Despite her prior awareness, Terri still found the video instructive, but in a different way than it was for Joy. As Terri explained, seeing the various settings in which racial discrimination occurred in the video expanded her existing understanding of the different forms that it can take. In her final interview, Terri identified ”True Colors” as one of the class sessions that she considered the most valuable. When I asked her to explain why she thought the video was valuable, she explained, 136 I think that one got the most response out of a lot of the people. . . . I think that the ”True Colors", you know, would show something to a lot of people. What impact would it have? Well, I think the realization that people are treated differently, and I think a lot of people, you know, they know that doesn't happen anymore and this and that, but that whole show was just total proof of whether it's still happening in our society today. [Teni 426-32,42] Interestingly, Terri appeared to feel that the primary value in the showing of the ”True Colors” video lay, not in any new knowledge or awareness that she herself might have gained from seeing it, but rather in the awareness that it might bring to some of her classmates, particularly those who contended that racial discrimination ”doesn't happen anymore." Terri obviously believes that discrimination is still a problem in this society, and she seems to want others to come to that realization as well. Whereas the video seemed to lead Joy to examine her own attitudes and actions, it prompted Terri, who already possessed favorable racial attitudes, to see it as a vehicle for possibly increasing the awareness among her classmates. In summary, the message of the "True Colors” video clashed with Claire's belief that everyone has equal opportunities in this society; therefore, she ascribed to it a meaning different than the one it was intended to convey. By interpreting "True Colors” as being about diversity, not discrimination, Claire was able to consider it one of the most valuable parts of the course and still hold on to her belief that everyone has equal opportunities. For Joy, the message of the video was easy to accept and remember because it reinforced her growing awareness of the problems of racism and discrimination. With this awareness she was beginning to recognize her own biases and to think about the implications that they would have for her as a teacher. Thus, viewing the video seemed to serve as an additional catalyst for growth. Finally, for Terri, the message of the video was a type of affirmation of something she was already well aware of through personal experience. In fact, she seemed to consider the central value of the video as lying in the fact that it made visible for others the reality of discrimination and, in so doing, might serve to increase their awareness. 137 Another class activity that illustrates the different ways that the three students responded would be the role-playing activities (see Appendix A) in which students engaged at different points during the semester. The role-playing activities dealt with issues like racism, sexism, inclusion, immigrants, sexual preference, abortion, and numerous others. For these activities students were typically divided into groups of four to five students. Each student was then given a slip of paper which briefly described the position he/she was to take on the particular issue that the group was to discuss. Generally, one of the five students would serve as an observer and would not participate in the discussion, but at the end of the discussion (usually no more than ten minutes), this observer would tell the other group members what he/she had observed. Depending on what other activities were scheduled for that particular class period and the amount of time available, there would generally be some whole-class discussion of what had occurred in the various groups. Claire. Claire did not have a positive reaction to the role-playing activities. In particular, she objected to having to assume someone else's position in a discussion. I like it as far as just reading through 'em and reading, like, this person has this view point and there's this one, but knowing that I have my own and how I would react is what I would rather do than to try and react as somebody else is going to, because going through and reading it, I know their opinion and so it just kinda», I don't know how to phrase it, but does that make sense? They have their own opinion and I have mine. I accept theirs whetherI agree with it or not. But you find it hard to argue their opinions? Well, not necessarily that, but I mean, I would rather go at it with my opinion and attempt to do or say something that I would in real life, under my opinion, knowing theirs and kinda, I guess you'd say "compromising,” rather than to know my opinion but try and act somebody else's opinion. I guess I would just rather be able to read it and know of this other opinion that's out there. [Claire 325-134] Although Claire was interested in reading about the opinions of others, she did not seem to see the value in taking on another person's perspective in discussing an issue. Apparently, despite having participated in the activity several times, she didn't seem to recognize that arguing a position different than her own could help her to understand that position more fully. Typically, the various positions were described only in 2-4 sentences on little slips of paper that the students were given, yet Claire asserted that 138 " going through and reading it, I know their opinion,” suggesting that perhaps she felt she already understood the positions just from these brief descriptions. Her later statement, "I would just rather be able to read it and know of this other opinion that's out there" offers further evidence that she may have been more interested in knowing about other positions than she was in understanding them; or, maybe in her mind, awareness of others' opinions was equivalent to understanding them. In her final interview Claire identified the role-playing activities as one of the least valuable aspects of the course, for essentially the same reasons as she had mentioned previously. I think I would have found it just as interesting to talk about it as a group and just kind of mention that, okay, this is an opinion and this is what they might think or this is how they might react whatever, but I’m always going to have my opinion, and like I’ve said, just to know the other opinions and other sides of things has been what’s come across most to me in this class, and so I--, I don’t know, I guess I found it not that important to play a different role, when I’m going to have my own [opinion], but just to go through and discuss the different sides rather than to play ‘em out. [Claire 429-44] Claire says, ”I'm always going to have my opinion," and it seems that she is reluctant to let go of her opinion, even for a few minutes during role-playing. Because she has her own opinion, she believes that it is ”not that important to play a different role.” She seems to be saying that because her opinion on the issue is already decided, she doesn't need to try to look at it from another perspective; she is merely interested in hearing what the other opinions are. Her comments give me the impression that her opinion is already set and being exposed to others' opinions isn't likely to change hers. Joy. Joy's reaction to the role-playing activities was quite different than Claire's. Rather than resisting the taking on of another person's perspective, Joy seemed to enjoy doing so. What really sticks out in my mind is when we do role-playing. I think that's so fascinating to take on a completely different opinion than my own. It kind of scares me because sometimes I can argue that opinion really well, and people are looking at me like, ”Does she really think that?" I mean, something horrible, like about racism or stuff, and then I have ask to myself, ”I made a lot of convincing arguments right there, you know. Maybe this isn't--," do you see what I'm saying? Like, maybe there is a little bit of prej», I know there's a little bit because 139 I think we all have prejudice, but I think there is prejudice in the back of my head. For me to be able to argue a situation so well actually kind of scared me, you know. [Joy 210-228] Joy didn't explain why she thought it was "so fascinating to take on a completely different opinion than my own,” but she obviously saw some value in doing so, whereas Claire apparently did not. The fact that Joy was able to argue a different opinion really well suggests that she had the ability to look at an issue from another's perspective. This being the case, I suspect that she was probably able to gain a better understanding of that perspective than was Claire. But for Joy, perhaps more important than any understanding that she may have gained of a different perspective may have been the insight that she gained into herself. She commented that her ability to play negative roles so well was rather frightening because she realized that that could indicate that she had some of those prejudiced thoughts in the back of her head already. Is it possible that the role-playing brought out your real feelings, and the rest of the time you're just pretending to be a certain way?) I'm sure it's possible. I'm sure some of the stuff that I said is in the back of my head. Do you know what I'm saying? Like some of the prejudice that I have--- Yeah, I think a truth comes out, you know, when you do that kind of stuff. [Joy 210-322] Joy acknowledged that some of the prejudiced views she expressed in the role-playing may actually have been her true feelings. Thus, for Joy, the role-playing provided an opportunity to learn not only about others but also about herself. In her final interview, Joy identified the role-playing activities as being one of the most valuable aspects of the course for her. Role-playing was because even though they weren't, like, their views, it just seemed [valuable] to try and work out different situations, you know, because maybe I don't have those views, but somebody out in that world is as negative as I was playing that role, and how do I deal with that person. That was that for me. [Joy 428-60] One value that Joy saw in the role-playing was that it provided some preparation for dealing with individuals who actually hold the views represented in the role-playing situations. Unlike Claire, who seems most interested in merely gaining an awareness of others' views, Joy seems to be interested in understanding others' views and how to deal 140 with them. Also unlike Claire, Joy says nothing that suggests that her views on the various issues are already set and not open to change. Terri. Although Terri talked about the role-playing activities only in the final interview, she also identified them as one of the most valuable parts of the course. I liked a lot of the different role-plays that we had to do, and I liked the discussions that we would get into in lab. My suggestion would probably be that sometimes, because you just presented it to your group, you would work on it and then, you know, end up doing whatever, but I think that if you had to get up in front of the whole class tha --, although there's a lot of people that would get nervous about it, but it would help with people not getting nervous 'cause you get to know the people in the class, but I think that also, you know, it would raise other people's consciousness, and when you're doing that in front of everyone, you're going to work hard to be your part and to discuss and to do things I think a little more. [Teni 426—24] Terri does not say so directly, but the fact that she liked the role-playing would suggest that she wasn't bothered by having to represent an opinion other than her own in the discussions. Terri's perception of the value of the role-playing activities was quite different from that of either Claire or Joy. And the role-playing, what was the value in that? In that, it got a lot of discussion and because it told you what to say on there whatever, too. A lot of those ideas were things that people wouldn't think of. I mean, they weren't ideas of like a lot of people, and to argue that was kind of hard, but then when you heard people talking about it and arguing the different sides, I think that it just brought out a lot of conversation, brought out a lot of things that people feel and then we got to talk through it. [Teni 426-38] In Terri's eyes, the greatest value in the role-playing activities was the discussion that it generated because it brought out people's feelings and gave them the opportunity to talk about them. As with the "True Colors” video, Terri seemed to be more interested in how the role-playing impacted her classmates than she was in its impact on herself. Again, I think this rrright be a reflection of the fact that she saw herself as being more racially aware than most of her classmates. Terri's comments on "True Colors” suggested that she wants to see her less aware classmates exposed to things that will increase their awareness of racial issues, and she apparently saw the role-playing activities as another vehicle for doing that 141 In summary, the three students' responses to the role-playing activities follow a pattern similar to that I observed with the ”True Colors” video. Once again, Claire seems to have missed the point of the activity. She did not appear to regard the role-playing as an opportunity to "walk a mile” in someone else's shoes and to gain a better understanding of others' perspectives. Claire stressed that she already had her own opinion on issues, and she apparently did not want to give up those opinions, even for a few minutes, to view the issues from another's perspective. As with the ”True Colors” video, she seemed reluctant to allow outside information to interfere with what she already believed. Of the three students, Joy, again, seemed to be the one who learned the most from the activity. Not only could she see value in taking on the perspectives of others through role-playing, she also gained some insight about herself. Like the ”True Colors” video, the role-playing activities helped to increase Joy's awareness of her own biases and probably served as another catalyst to her growth. Although Terri did not explain any of her personal reactions to the role-playing, she did view it as one of the most valuable activities in the course. Once again, however, she seemed to see their major value as lying in the fact that they might serve to increase the awareness of some of her classmates. The Influence of Personal anljties While this study was centered on how racial attitudes and beliefs influence what students learn from a course on diversity, it became apparent to me during my analysis of the interviews for the three focus students that certain personal qualities also seemed to play an important role in determining how they responded to the course content. Specifically, I noticed that these three students seemed to differ in terms of their openness and their ability to be self -reflective. In this discussion I define openness as being receptive to others' ideas or arguments (i.e., open-minded) and also as being accepting of diversity. I define self-reflective as having an awareness of one's own beliefs and 142 attitudes, as well as being willing and/or able to think critically about them. While these two qualities are different, they are related, sol will discuss them together. Claire. Although she considered herself to be open-minded, in my opinion Claire actually showed little evidence of openness. In our third interview I asked her to explain what she meant when she said she was open-minded. I need to get a sense of what you mean when you say you're open-minded. I feel that everybody has an equal opportunity, that, you know, if maybe somebody is Black and maybe somebody is White. It's a physical difference, but we still both put our pants on one leg at a time. As far as anything else, like, you can walk around on campus and see maybe somebody has had half their head shaved and half of it's not. Well, if that's what they want, then so be it. It's not something I would choose to have done, but as long as it's not disruptive to other people or something to that effect, if someone wants to do something like that, that's their choice. [Claire 225-70] She seemed to understand the meaning of open-minded, and there were occasions in the interviews when she made comments that seemed consistent with being open-minded. For example, in our fourth interview she commented, I have picked up a lot from this class as far as seeing situations, seeing other people's opinions and vieWpoints on the subject, other than my own, and whether or not I agree with them, I accept them because they're theirs, but it also lets me see the other side. [Claire 325-134] Nevertheless, despite occasional remarks like the one above, her interviews overall suggest that she was not at all open to ideas that conflicted with her own. The role- playing activities in CEP 240 provided students with opportunities to further their understanding of different perspectives on various diversity issues and to reflect on and perhaps clarify their own beliefs. As I explained in the previous section of this chapter, Claire resisted the role-playing activities because they required her to take on a perspective different than her own. I’m always going to have my opinion, and like I’ve said, just to know the other opinions and other sides of things has been what’s come across most to me in this class, and so I don’t know; I guess I found it not that important to play a different role, when I’m going to have my own [opinion], but just to go through and discuss the different sides rather than to play ‘em out. [Claire 429-44] 143 Apparently, Claire wanted to hear about other perspectives, but she did not want to look at an issue from one of those perspectives, though doing so might have enhanced her understanding of that perspective. She seemed to believe that because she already had her opinion on the issue (an opinion that, apparently, was not open to change), there was no need for her to try to understand others' opinions; awareness of those opinions seemed to be sufficient for her. It is possible that Claire's distaste for role-playing, in and of itself, had nothing to do with her openness or her self-reflectiveness; however, the weight of other evidence suggests otherwise. In analyzing Claire's interviews, I noticed a number of instances where being exposed to new ideas or ideas that conflicted with her own led her, not to reappraise and reflect on, but to reiterate and defend what she already believed. For example, during our fifth interview I asked Claire whether or not she believed there were advantages to being White in this society, and we spent a good portion of that interview discussing this question. Claire answered that she didn't think that Whites had any advantages. I then asked her to consider the question from another perspective: Try to imagine someone who would argue that there are advantages to being White. What would they say would be some of the benefits? I think maybe it's not a benefit but I think more expectation would be one because out in society your majority have the power and so you should be able to do whatever. I think being minority actually has better benefits as far as society goes because they've got their scholarships--Latino and Black, and there's scholarships for White but not just a White person. I mean, from what I've seen, and I haven't seen 'em all, but if a White person can apply for a scholarship, anybody can, but there's specified scholarships and stuff for just Blacks or just Latino or whatever, so as far as being White, I think that would be a disadvantage in society, but then they have the higher expectations so they're expecting you to be able to make more money and not need the scholarship or whatever. [Claire 412—244] Rather than identifying possible advantages to being White, Claire instead asserted that minorities have more benefits than Whites. Her response suggests to me that she was either unable or unwilling to identify benefits to being White in this society. Once again she seemed to have her opinion on the issue, and she didn't seem willing to entertain an alternative perspective. Later in the same interview I showed Claire an article from the 144 local newspaper which pointed out the discrepancies in the average annual incomes for Whites and non-Whites in the local metr0politan area. Looking at this article from the [local newspaper], how would you explain the dtfi‘erence in the average income of Whites and minorities in the [metropolitan] area? I don't think it's because there isn't an equal chance. I think everybody has an equal opportunity, and especially now and within the last couple years because they've become more aware of not hiring somebody because they're Black or whatever. Maybe it is a White person in a higher paid position. I mean, there's several reasons I'm sure, but maybe the Hispanic or Black whatever didn't want that position. . . . I don't think it has anything to do with being Black, Hispanic, or being White. I think a lot of it has to do with what they want to pursue. [Claire 412-298] When confronted with statistics that would seem to contradict her idea that everyone has equal opportunities, Claire, rather than reconsidering her belief, attempted to explain the statistics away by suggesting that the explanation might lie within the individuals themselves. She suggested that maybe Blacks and Hispanics didn't want the hi ghet- paying jobs. As we discussed the income differences further, Claire later commented, ”I don't know why there's a difference. Maybe Whites have more motivation. As far as why, I don't know." [Claire 412—381]. She steadfastly refused to reconsider her notion that everyone has equal opportunities, preferring instead to attribute the differences to Whites being more motivated than minorities. (I find it interesting that, on the one hand, Claire suggested that race didn't matter, but on the other hand, she argued that Whites may be more motivated.) I continued to push Claire by showing her another article with statistics on unemployment in the area. This article shows unemployment in the [metropolitan [area. Do you have any theories as to why there are more Blacks and Hispanics unemployed than there are Whites? I think if I had seen that for, like, ten years ago, I would have said it was because of the problem with not employing somebody because they are Black or because they are Hispanic. As far as now, I have no idea. I don't think it's because of lack of motivation, although there are people, Black and White and Hispanic and everybody, who don't have the motivation. They just don't want to do anything, but it's not because of their race. That's just them. I don't think it's lack of opportunity. I mean, anybody could get a part-time job, I'm sure, flopping burgers. . . . As far as why there's more Blacks and Hispanics unemployed, I don't know, but there's a variety of reasons for unemployment, and it's not because of race or color. [Claire 412-427] 145 The first part of Claire's response indicates that she believed that racial discrimination was a thing of the past, and apparently she was still not willing to re-examine that belief in light of the statistical data that I had shown her. In my mind, the preceding excerpts provide clear evidence of Claire's lack of openness to conflicting ideas and her unwillingness to be critical of her own beliefs and attitudes. However, there is additional evidence. Eariier in this chapter I explained how, for Claire, the showing of the "True Colors" video did not push her to re-examine her beliefs about everyone having equal opportunities in this country. Rather than critically re-evaluating her belief in light of this new information, she appeared to reconstruct the meaning of video to make it consistent with her existing belief. As further evidence, one of Claire's comments during our second interview suggests that in her lab section she may have been exposed to conflicting opinions which she also either failed to acknowledge or perhaps just misinterpreted. There is a lot of diversity, just in our room alone, which makes it really interesting because then you can get both sides of any story, seeing how, for example, the Black people feel like they're being discriminated against, and the White people, as well», just getting both sides of the story, which was really interesting. Besides being interesting, is there any other reason why that seems important? Just to show that, I mean, everybody has the same opportunities. It's available to anyone, I guess. [Claire 211-257] I was not present in Claire's lab section; however, if the Black students in her class were complaining about being discriminated against, it is unlikely that they also felt that everyone has equal opportunities. Yet, in some mysterious way, Claire seemed to interpret what these students had to say as being consistent with her belief that everyone has equal opportunities. Claire may have heard both sides of the story, but it doesn't appear as though hearing the other side had any impact on her beliefs, except maybe to reinforce them. Again I think she demonstrated either an inability or an unwillingness to open up her existing beliefs and attitudes to critical re-examination when she was confronted with conflicting information. 146 While Claire seemed to believe that she was open to diversity, some of her comments suggest otherwise. During our second interview, Claire offered the following remark: There's one thing that, I guess, really bothers me. I don't have like a great all-time solution, but we've been talking about diversity and everything, like the story we read for class about the Indian. . . . okay, this Indian comes into class. Well, then they have to start changing everything, so ”Oh, okay, let's have a day for everybody to have their own holiday and let's do this so that everybody's included.” . . . sometimes, and this is kinda mean, but sometimes I'm glad I'm in math because everybody deals with numbers; they're straight across the board and I don't have to worry about, you know, Egyptian numbers or (laughs) [inaudible] numbers. It's just numbers, and so sometimes in that respect I'm kind of glad that I'm there. [Claire 211-494] Claire's apparent concern about having to change things in order to accommodate people who are different seems to suggest that she regarded celebrating diversity as a problem that she'd rather not have to deal with. She went on to explain that she thought it would be difficult to balance between those who want to include others and those who want to leave others out. Though it may not have been her intention, this explanation suggests that she would make changes in her classroom primarily to please others and to avoid controversy, not out of any personal belief in acknowledging and celebrating diversity. Her insinuation that diversity would not be an issue in teaching math prompted me to ask her another question: Don't diversity issues come up in mathematics? I know you're dealing with numbers, but you 're also dealing with people. Oh, this is true. When I become a teacher, I don't want to become, like, next door neighbor, best-friend buddies with every single student, but you know, to be a part of their life maybe as a coach or just a friend that they can come talk to or something, so there'll be differences but still in that fact it's not going to mean any difference in the way I teach. It'll just make it interesting because I'll have a variety of backgrounds and cultures et cetera. (Claire, 211-547] In essence, Claire seemed to think that having diverse learners in her classroom would be interesting, but it would have no effect on her instruction. She went on to explain that ”I wouldn't treat 'em any different or think that they're any less of a person, although it might take more one on one or something" [Claire 21 1-571]. This final comment subtly suggests that she thought diverse students were likely to be academically deficient and to 147 require extra attention. In her final interview Claire made another comment which leads me to question her openness to diversity . . . Black people have, as far as I know, the exact same rights as we do. I mean, they can vote; they have freedom of speech, etcetera, you know, and it’s almost to the point of where I think, “What else do you want?” [Claire 429-104] If Claire feels that racial minorities already have equal opportunities and are asking for too much, she is less likely to see the need for or to be supportive of efforts to promote diversity. In addition to Claire's lack of open-mindedness and her lack of openness to diversity, there was also evidence that she was not very aware of her own racial attitudes and beliefs. Numerous times during the semester she made statements that people were people to her and that their race didn't matter, but she also made a couple of shocking statements which seemed to contradict those assertions of equality. Specifically, she hypothesized that one reason for the lower school achievement of Blacks may be that they are genetically inferior. On another occasion she suggested that one reason the average income of Whites is higher than that of Blacks and Hispanics is that Whites are more motivated. There were also numerous instances when she voiced more subtle stereotypes that also contradicted her claims of equality, including her apparent assumption that minority children come from problem—filled home environments, that they will need more help academically, and her well-meaning observation that ”any Black person can be just as good as a White person" [Claire 211-457]. Given the preceding evidence of some of Claire's stereotypical beliefs about racial minorities, the following comment from the final interview seems to highlight her lack of self-awareness in this respect. I don’t try to be prejudiced, and if I am, I don’t realize it, but I mean, maybe I am. Maybe I’m just this total hypocrite, you know; I’m just flying off the wall here, but I don’t tlrinkl am, and I don’t realize it if I am. [Claire 429-130] Though I searched for disconfirrning evidence, I could find few instances in the interviews where her exposure to new or conflicting ideas about racial issues led Claire to 148 reflect on and reevaluate her existing attitudes and beliefs. (It is possible, of course, that she did her questioning and re-examining outside of the interviews, but frankly, I feel that that is unlikely.) One instance occurred during the second interview, which took place shortly after the class had viewed the "True Colors” video. Claire offered the following initial reaction to the video: I really didn't think there was that much difference out in the real world. I kind of sat there in shock. . . . I guess I just didn't realize that it was that different, you know, that they're treated that different. But you saw evidence, didn't you? Yeah, this is true. Unfortunately, it obviously happens. [Claire 211-367,385] The impact of the video, however, seemed to be short-lived, for as I reported earlier in this chapter, within a few weeks Claire had somehow reconstructed the message of the video so that it no longer illustrated racial discrimination and, hence, no longer conflicted with her deeply-held belief that everyone has equal opportunities in this society. In another instance, when Claire expressed the belief that the ratio of majority students to minority students on campus was about 50-50, I corrected her by saying the ratio was more like, at best, 80-20. She responded, "I thought it was close to 50-50, you know, without any numbers or anything. I guess it's not So I guess in that aspect there isn't as much diversity as there should be” [Claire 412-184]. Claire expressed mild surprise, but she seemed to have no difficulty accepting these figures. I suspect that the reason Claire _let go of her belief so easily was that, because she was only guessing about the majority to minority ratio, it was not a belief that she strongly held. One other instance when Claire responded positively to a conflicting idea occurred during our fifth interview. In this interview we had a long discussion about whether or not there were advantages to being White in this society. At one point we had the following exchange: Aren't there a lot of difierent ways that there are advantages to being White because you 're constantly reinforced, but if you 're over here, you're either put down or you 're not as visible, so you don 't feel good about yourself? Yeah, I guess, I think it is possible. (Pause) As far as in that aspect, I think it's a lot on the student and the child's part to», okay, they're gonna know that they're in the minority, but they've got to look at it that it's the other person's problem if they feel they have to pick on me, the minority child whatever. I think a lot of it is just going to be ignoring it and realizing that they are just as good as the next person. 149 Unfortunately, this may put extra burden on the minority child, but again, it doesn't mean they have any less of an opportunity. . . . [Claire 412-004] While she initially seemed to accept the plausibility of the idea being presented to her, she then proceeded to try to explain it away and to show me that it didn't conflict with her belief that everyone has equal opportunities, even though it seems rather contradictory to say on the one hand that minority children will have an extra burden, but then on the other hand say that this extra burden doesn't result in any less opportunity for them. While I have argued that Claire showed little awareness of and little ability to reflect critically on her attitudes and beliefs, that is not to say she was completely lacking in these respects. For example, she demonstrated some self -awareness on one occasion when I asked her to compare herself to a person in an article that the class had read. In what ways do you see yourself as being similar to Vanessa, and in what ways do you see yourself as being different? As far as being similar, she said that it doesn't matter if they're Black or White or whatever. I feel the same way. It doesn't matter what somebody is, but yet you've got to be able to realize that, like, you are Black and your culture is different, so not to hide», to be able to celebrate your diversity but also to be able to say that you're just as equal as I am. I feel that's how I'm similar. As far as being different, as far as I know, I try not to use the contradicting terms that she did. "Well, I'm not prejudiced, but the Blacks do this." Well, you know, for instance, Blacks maybe are poor. There's White people that are poor, too. There's Black people that are rich; White people»- Without realizing it, you know, I don't think I do, but maybe I do. [Claire 412-205] Evidence that I have cited earlier suggests that Claire was guilty of the same contradictory behavior of which she accused Vanessa (e. g., saying that people's color doesn't matter but also saying that Blacks are genetically inferior to Whites). Interestingly, however, she expressed an awareness that she might be behaving in that fashion even though she tried not to. In other words, she seemed to be aware that she might not be aware of some of her own prejudices. During the final interview Claire demonstrated some ability to reflect critically on her attitudes and beliefs. I try to give everybody an equal chance and, you know, everybody has an equal opportunity in general anyways, but I think also I’ve said that sometimes you don’t realize you’re doing something unless it’s pointed out to you. There were times where I would think of something to say, and then I’d think, well, is that prejudiced, you know? I don’t know. I guess maybe I just got to questioning myself because, you know, we’ve just been discussing it. . . . [Claire 429-130] 150 The above passage suggests that Claire did sometimes engage in self -questioning and critical evaluation of her ideas. Thus, Claire did show some evidence of openness and self -reflectiveness; however, in both of these qualities she was far behind Joy and Terri. Joy. In my interviews with Joy throughout the semester, there was substantial evidence of her openness to others' ideas. For example, at one point during our second interview Joy expressed the belief that the media plays up the negative aspects of Detroit because a large percentage of the people who live there are Black. What makes you think that that's the reason? I think our society, like, sometimes tries to blame a lot of things that go wrong on Black people, for some reason. What makes you think that? I'm studying a lot right now about, like, the slave trade and all that, like going back to early colonial times, and I think a lot of that "the White man thinks they're better than the Black man” is still here, and it's called prejudice, and I think it's still there and I think it's a scapegoat for White people to blame it on Black people because, I mean,obviously the only difference is the color of our skin. I think White people honestly feel that they are better than Black people. . . . [Joy 210-65,68] When asked to explain what was, to me, a rather surprising insight on her part, Joy made reference to what she was learning in another course, thus indicating that she had embraced at least some of the information that was presented to her in the course and that this information had become part of her understanding of why things are they way they are. A second example of Joy's openness to others' ideas can be found in her response to the "True Colors” video. . . . to me it's just fascinating how in this day and age how everything's supposed to be so equal and equality blah, blah, blah, how things are not equal. . . . .It was more powerful the first time I saw it, but it was really neat to like, to see people in my class going, "I can't believe this really happens,” you know. It was really, really fascinating to me. [Joy 210-228] Unlike Claire, Joy had seen the video before, but she still found it ”fascinating." Also unlike Claire, Joy did not reconstruct the central message of the video with the passage of time. In her final interview Joy said that she considered the video valuable because "it was a realization for me of how prejudiced our society is" [Joy 428-60]. Apparently, the video introduced Joy to new information, and she was open to it A third example of 151 Joy's openness occurred during our fifth interview. When I questioned her assertion that a White person walking down the street in a Black neighborhood would be discriminated against, Joy explained her reasoning: My thing is majority rule, and in any situation, like, when you have a group of people who get together and they're all the same, they're powerful, and they know they're powerful, and they can walk on people who aren't powerful. When the first White people came here, your pilgrims, they were in the minority. The Indians were very friendly to those people and did not stomp on them, so is it really true that just because you 're in the majority that you 're necessarily going to step on people who are less? When Columbus came, they welcomed him. Yeah, but maybe that's just the nature», I don't know. I don't know. That's a good point. It totally contradicts what I just said. [Joy 407-128,l36] While there were many occasions in our interviews where Joy would argue her position, the above exchange illustrates that she remained open-minded in doing so. She was willing to listen to and seriously consider ideas that clearly conflicted with her own. In addition to being open to others' ideas, Joy also indicated that she was open to diversity. During our first interview she commented, . . . you have to realize that everybody's different and because they believe in something different than you doesn't make them wrong; it doesn't make them horrible people whatever; that's just their views. I mean, what I think we need to teach our children is that being different is okay. [Joy 128-34] It was very obvious to me, from other things that Joy said in the interviews, that she was genuinely interested in being exposed to and learning about people who were different For example, in the second interview she explained why she thought it was important to have experiences with people of different backgrounds. I think the real reason I'm at Michigan State is because it's culturally diverse, and I feel that that's what the real worid is going to be. I told you last time about the two high schools that were in my district One was extremely culturally diverse, . and I had the choice whether I could go to that one or go to a completely all- White, very upper-class school, and I chose the diversity one. When I was in eighth grade and I made that decision, it was the same decision I had to make going here. I looked at the schools, smaller schools, small Catholic schools, like, just because of the range, like, five or six thousand people. I'd like the small classes and the individual attention, but I realized that that's not how the real world's going to be. Michigan State, for me, with the diversity and the larger classes, as much as I hate 'em, that's how the real world is gonna be. You know, you're not gonna always have the individual attention. You're going to need to cope with struggling with that How's this going to prepare you for it? 152 How's it going to prepare me? Because if I have to learn how to deal with different types of people now, it's only going to benefit me with my career, like as a teacher. If I'm learning to deal with people, like for example, on my floor where I live, there are Black people, there are Indian people, there are Chinese people. If I'm interacting with these people on a daily basis, it's only going to help me when I get out into the real worid because I 've had situations before where», it's like if I didn't, I wouldn't know what to expect I would maybe have, like, classifications in my head of people and maybe prejudge. [Joy 210-001, 021] Through the rest of our interviews, I really did not see any evidence that would lead me to question the sincerity of her openness to diversity. In fact, at one point she expressed disappointment that there was not more interaction between races at the university. She believed that interracial contact was an important key to improving interracial understanding. I think by working with 'em that that dissolves the stereotypes. I think if I didn't work with 'em, then those stereotypes would always be there. Like, unless you have an experience to go off of, why would your mind ever change? Do you see what I'm saying? Like, if I learn something, if I work with someone, then I'm going to be more apt to realize that that was a stereotype in my head and that people really aren't like that, you know. [Joy 210—448] On many different occasions Joy also demonstrated considerable awareness of her own beliefs and attitudes, as well as the ability to think critically about them. For example, during our second interview, she showed surprising insight when she made the following observation: What really sticks out in my mind is when we do role-playing. I think that's so fascinating to take on a completely different opinion than my own. It kind of scares me because sometimes I can argue that opinion really well, and people are looking at me like, "Does she really think that?" I mean, something horrible, like about racism or stuff, and then I have ask to myself. You know, I made a lot of convincing arguments right there. Maybe this isn't», do you see what I'm saying? Like, maybe there is a little bit of prej», I know there's a little bit because I think we all have prejudice, but I think there is prejudice in the back of my head. For me to be able to argue a situation so well actually kind of scared me, you know. [Joy 210—228] Joy recognized that perhaps one reason she was able to argue a racist's perspective so well was that she might actually hold some of those negative racial feelings herself. Unlike Claire, who appeared to be completely unaware that she possessed any type of negative racial attitude or belief, Joy expressed awareness of her racial prejudice. 153 Is it possible that the role-playing brought out your real feelings, and the rest of the time you 're just pretending to be a certain way? I'm sure it's possible. I'm sure some of the stuff thatl said is in the back of my head. Do you know what I'm saying? Like some of the prejudice that I have. Yeah, I mean, I think a truth comes out, you know, when you do that kind of stuff. [Joy 210—322] Instead of responding defensively to my question, as you might expect that she would, Joy once again demonstrated her openness to other's ideas as well'as her willingness to be self-critical by acknowledging this possibility. Another example of Joy's self -awareness comes from our fifth interview, when she gave an example of one of her prejudices. . . . like I noticed like in my dorm, for example, I get really annoyed at», I don't know if this is racist or whatever, but how typically loud the Black girls are in our hall, and I don't know that you could say that's racist or it's just me being, I don't know, anal sometimes I guess because I'm trying to study or whatever, but there are little things like that that maybe I guess I am because they're like a little racist or whatever, because I don't know if I-», okay, for example, there's like loud music going on like right across the hall from me, and there's two White girls, and [then] there's a huge, like, a bunch of girls, Black girls that are around, and they're all talking really, really loud and stuff, and it's the Black girls who I will get more like a feeling of ”Pleeasse be quiet!” than it's the White girls, and I don't know why that is. I don't I mean, I think that's racially, like, inside of me for some reason. Idon't know. [Joy 407-335] In my eyes, Joy shows great honesty and a lot of self-insight here. Not only was she aware that she had prejudices, she was able to clearly articulate one of them. In contrast, Claire was not even aware that she had prejudices and, therefore, certainly was not able to articulate what they were. Yet another example of Joy's self -insight comes from the same interview. So, is there a secret place down in you somewhere where you 're racist? No, I don't think that I'm racist, but I think I have had some racial imprints or something put in me, like, just when I was little and hearing my grandfather say something. I mean, that sticks in your head, you know. . . . Give me an example of something that was implanted in you as you were growing a up. Like my grandpa, he's so bad, like when I was a little girl, he'd be talking or whatever, and every time he'd refer to Black people, he would say, ”Burrhead," and sol never knew that was wrong until I said it to my mom one time, and she said, "What did you just say?!” And she explained to me that that is completely rude and wrong, and I just all of a sudden, I mean, I was ten years old and saying that word, not knowing that it was anything different than saying "a Black person,” you know, so there's little stuff like that that you learn just by communicating with your elders or whatever, that they may have racial views and they express 'em, and not being able to differentiate whether that's good or that's bad, you just kind of take on those views, you know. [Joy 407-335360] 154 This was one of many occasions in our interviews where Joy surprised me with her insight. She was aware that she possessed racial prejudices, she could recognize some of her prejudices, and she also had some understanding of how those prejudices probably originated. In addition, as explained earlier in this section of the chapter, she had reasonable ideas for how to combat her prejudices»through more interaction with individuals from different racial backgrounds. One final quotation from Joy's interviews illustrates both her openness to new ideas and her ability to be self -reflective. Has talking with me this semester affected either your perceptions of the course or the way you think about any of the issues covered in the course? Definitely. I think it's made me more aware about my views on different races and different subject areas. You've made me think a lot about myself and just my [beliefs], you know, like, what I thought. I mean, before this class I never thought I was prejudiced. I was like, "I'm not. What are you talking about?" But I, like, little stuff you made me realize that, yeah, like, I do have those feelings inside me that I don't know [inaudible], like, grotesque or whatever, but», (brief pause) Yeah, I've learned a lot [Joy 428-189] Although I could cite many other examples, it should be pretty clear that Joy was much more aware of her racial attitudes and beliefs than was Claire, and she also showed much greater ability to look at her attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors critically. Terri. In terms of her openness and self -reflectiveness, Terri was much more like Joy than Claire. There were several ways in which she demonstrated her openness to other's ideas. First, I consider being able to see the other side of an issue as a type of openness, and this was an ability that Terri demonstrated. During our third interview, Terri expressed her opposition to affirmative action scholarships. After some discussion, I asked her a question to determine whether or not she had any knowledge of the other side of the issue. What are the reasons why some people advocate aflirmative action? Well, I think that a lot of it is that everybody's so sensitive about things right now and so sensitive about how people were treated in the past and how people are continued to be treated, and I think that a lot of times people are trying to make amends, you know, and saying now, "Well, we'll give you this special scholarship and this and that to show you that we aren't going to discriminate against you and we aren't», this isn't going to happen and blah, blah, blah, in order to bring that person into that place, and I think that's a lot of times what happens. [I‘erri 314-055] 155 Unlike Claire, who seemed either unable or unwilling to see an issue from the opposite perspective, Terri demonstrated some understanding of the other side of the issue. Another example of her openness occurred during the first interview, when Terri indicated to me that the information being presented on racism and discrimination was not new to her. . . . a lot of the stuff that we've talked», I guess it really hasn't been that much of a new experience to me, but I like to see other people because a lot of that stuff is so new to them, and a lot of the ideas and a lot of different things that we're talking about, I mean, talking about minority and talking about different things. I mean, to me it isn't really that different, but I can see that in the future a lot that we're talking about is in a lot of areas, such as being [blind] and deaf and different things like that, [that] I'm not very familiar with, and I'm kind of intrigued to learn about it and maybe even follow into the special education field. [Terri 131-193] Given Terri's prior experiences with racism and discrimination, it would have been easy for her to take the attitude that the class was boring and a waste of her time, but instead she looked forward to learning about issues with which she was less familiar. In a similar vein, Terri's response to her viewing of the "True Colors" video also showed a type of openness. Not only had she seen the video previously, she knew from personal experience that racial discrimination was still a problem in this society. Nevertheless, she was still able to find value in watching the video again, as she explained, I just think that a lot of the different varieties that [the racial discrimination] came in»at the store and this and that, so I can see a store and I can understand. I've seen that before, so that wasn't really anything new, but even the car salesman, the man himself who was Black and didn't rush out there either, and the different prices and when they were confronted about it It's just a lot of different things. I mean, I've never gone and priced a car, so I'm not», I've never seen that before, so it's really just a lot of the different areas that you found it in. [Terri 217-342] Again, Terri does not seem to take the attitude that she knows all that she needs to know; she clearly seems to be open to learning more. In our firml interview, I found a third example of Terri's openness. What if I told you after these interviews and so on that I came to the conclusion that you were prejudiced? I'd listen to what you had to say, but (laughs) I probably would feel a little offended, you know. I'd want to know why you would say that. [Terri 426-177] 156 Though Terri had never said anything to give me the impression that she was racially prejudiced, I had not shared this impression with Teni, and I was curious to see how she would respond to the above question. I think it is noteworthy that her first inclination would be to listen to my reasons for thinking that she was prejudiced. To me, this is another example of her openness; I think that many, if not most, people would probably react defensively or maybe just ignore whatever I had to say because they know that they're not prejudiced. Teni didn't consider herself prejudiced, but apparently she would still be open to listening to my reasons for thinking she was prejudiced. In my interviews with Terri there is abundant evidence of her openness to diversity. She was certainly one of the most open and most accepting individuals that I talked with. I think part of her openness was attributable to her many experiences with people from a variety of different backgrounds. My friends are from all different backgrounds, all different races, and when I sit in the class and when we talk in our lab, I hear people saying, "I've never met anyone that's different than me. My school was all White. My school was all this.” Well, my school was very diverse. I Ieamed all that», I mean, I just think that a lot of the experiences I've had and knowing what to do and what not to do, I've learned definitely not to stereotype people, and I see so many people doing it here. It's because they don't know at all, and I think that I've been educated well enough. I mean, I just think that I have had enough experiences right now that a lot of these things we read about, a lot of these things I have already known, I've already experienced, but there's a lot of people in the class that are looking at this for the first time, and they're just thinking, ”Oh, my gol.‘ It's so new to them, but to me it's not new at all. [Terri 131-60] Based on the preceding excerpt and on other comments that she made about various experiences that she had had, it was obvious that Teni did more than just express an openness to diversity; she actually seemed to live it, and one of her comments in the final interview illustrates that I had asked Terri to explain why she thought she was part of the solution to the problem of racism and discrimination. She responded, I think that by my actions and by the things I do, I'm an example to people, and I never let a person ever get away with ever saying anything that's racist or anything around me. I will always make sure I say something; I don't ignore it, and a lot of people would, you know, who would say, ”I'm not racist, I'm not this whatever," and would laugh at some kind of a joke or something, but I think that everything about me just shows that I'm not like that, sol think that by being an 157 example to whomever it may be, I mean, people here, people there, whoever knows me or something, I think that that is kind of part of a solution. [Terri 426-131] In my mind, Terri's actions signify a true commitment to respecting diversity. I am impressed with her recognition that ignoring or laughing at racist comments is a way of condoning those comments, and she will have no part of that because she wants to make sure that people know that she disapproves of such comments. In addition to her openness, Terri also showed her self -reflectiveness through her awareness of her own attitudes and beliefs along with her ability to think critically about them. An example of Terri's self -awareness appeared in our second interview. I had asked her how she could be unprejudiced, as she had claimed, after growing up in a community with prejudiced people all around her. Her response was as follows: Well, obviously, I mean, my parents, they were prejudiced. They had a lot of prejudice in 'em, and my grandfather, oh my gol! I can't even say one thing to him. He's from the upper peninsula, so, I mean, he's just horrible, but I think that it kinda comes within yourself. I mean, within myself I just», I guess it's just something that I've done myself to learn to see. I've seen all the injustice. I've heard my friends talk about different things that have happened to them and a lot of different things like that, and I just see that I could never be like that and that I would never want to do that to anyone, and that I find it, like, rewarding. I mean, a lot of people don't reach out and have friends from cultural and ethnic different backgrounds whatever, but I enjoy having people. I don't want somebody who's exactly like me. That would be so boring, to have someone exactly like me. . . . [Terri 217-095] When I have asked other students this question, some of them have had difficulty coming up with an appropriate response. Terri, however, showed what I consider to be a type of self-awareness when she was able to provide at least a partial response, although I'm sure there were other factors involved of which she was unaware. She seemed to be saying that despite the presence of prejudiced attitudes around her, she had made a conscious decision not to adopt those attitudes Plus, it seems apparent that having a number of friends from different racial backgrounds was instrumental in helping her come to understand the effects of racial prejudice and discrimination. One example of her ability to think critically about her attitudes and beliefs appeared during our second interview as 158 well. Terri had commented that some of her ideas were changing, and I asked her to elaborate. Well, the issue that we talked about concerning the answer that I gave with the kids in the class speaking Black English, and I was saying that I would expect them to speak proper English, and I now realize, you know, that that was kind of a very shallow answer for myself, and through a lot of the reading and a lot of the examples that we've done in class, too, even today in discussion, I see that there really is no basic, standard English language. It's just, you know, what I myself have perceived and so I think I've kind of made amends on that feeling. [Teni 217-220] I think this excerpt shows that Terri was reflective, in that after the interview in which we discussed the issue of Black English, she continued to think about the issue and her position on it. This excerpt also shows that Terri was able to be critical of her own attitudes and beliefs. Additionally, I think it serves as another illustration of her openness to new ideas because, as she explained, the readings and other experiences in the class led her to change her position on the use of Black English. Another good example of her self -reflectiveness occurred during our final interview. Has talking with me this semester afi'ected either your perceptions of the course or the way you think about any of the issues covered in the course? Yeah, yes, I would have to say. I don't know what it is but sometimes, like, talking to you, I'll say whatever first comes to my mind, and I'll just get all mixed up with what I'm saying, and I won't really think about it, and then I'll get up to my room and I'll think, "What was I saying? Why did you answer it that way?" or whatever, and for some reason, I mean, it'll be on my mind for a couple of days or whatever, and I'll just be like, ”How could I have answered that?“ I kind of work it out in my head, you know, what I was trying to say or what I was meaning and stuff, but I do think sometimes it really challenged me, you know, of what to and what not to say, and I don't know, I would always be thinking, "No, that wasn't the right answer," you know; "I should have said this or that," sol think it did challenge me. [Terri 426—151] In this excerpt Terri illustrated that she often critically reflected on what she had said during her interviews with me, questioning the things that she had said, pondering what her ideas really were, and trying to think of ways that she might express them more clearly. In summary, the qualitative data reported in this section cleariy suggest that Claire, the focus student who demonstrated the least movement toward developing more favorable racial attitudes and beliefs, was also the student who demonstrated the least 159 openness and the least self -reflectiveness. On the other hand, Terri, who began the course with very favorable racial attitudes, and Joy, who seemed to be moving in the direction of developing more favorable attitudes, both demonstrated considerable openness to diversity and to new or conflicting ideas, as well as a high degree of self - reflectiveness, particularly self-awareness and the ability to be self-critical. Thus, it appears that the personal qualities of openness and self -reflectiveness may be important variables determining how students' respond to courses on diversity, and these qualities may need to be taken into consideration along with students' entering racial attitudes and beliefs. CHAPTER FIVE SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS In this study I investigated whether preservice teachers' racial attitudes and beliefs appeared to be related to how they perceived and what they Ieamed from a course on diversity. Specifically, I sought to determine 1) how students who entered the course with more favorable racial attitudes and beliefs were similar to and different from those who entered with less favorable racial attitudes and beliefs, 2) whether students' racial attitudes and beliefs appeared to change during the semester, and 3) whether students with more favorable racial attitudes and beliefs would experience the course differently than those with less favorable attitudes and beliefs. In this chapter I will summarize and discuss the major findings from my analysis of the quantitative and qualitative data, I will discuss some of the implications that these findings would seem to have for our on-going efforts to prepare teachers for diversity, I will identify some of the limitations of the present study, and finally, I will suggest possible directions for future research in this area. Summary of Major [findings The major findings of this study were derived from both quantitative and qualitative data. I will summarize separately the findings from each type of data. Quantigtive Data The quantitative data sources were the attitude survey, the teaching/learning questionnaire, and course records of attendance, exam scores, and course grades. My analysis of these data yielded five major findings: 160 161 1. A comparative analysis of students' responses to the individual items on the Quick Discrimination Index (QDI) revealed that students who scored high differed significantly in the following major ways from those who scored low : a) Perceptions of minorities. Students who scored high were more likely than those who scored low to regard themselves as unprejudiced, and they appeared to hold more positive images of racial minorities. b) Recognition of racism and discrimination. Those scoring high appeared to be more aware of and more knowledgeable about the racism and discrimination that exists in our society. c) Friendships with minorities. Students who scored high reported having greater quantity and higher quality friendships with people who are racially different than themselves. <1) View on interracial relationships. Students who scored high appeared to be much more accepting of interracial dating and marriage than were students who scored low. e) Living near or working with minorities. Students who scored high expressed dramatically more positive feelings toward living in a diverse neighborhood and also toward teaching in a diverse school. 0 Sensitivity to minority issues. Students who scored high appeared to be more sensitive to minority concerns about racism and discrimination than were students who scored low. g) View on multicultural education. Students who scored high were more likely to support the idea of multicultural education than were students who scored low. This very strong and consistent pattern of differences between students who scored high and those who scored low on the QDI suggest that higher scores on the QDI may be reasonably regarded as indicators of more favorable racial attitudes, while lower scores may be regarded as indicators of less favorable racial attitudes. 162 2. Among students in the volunteer group, those who began the course with more favorable racial attitudes apparently moved toward having even more favorable attitudes by the end of the course; in contrast, students who began with less favorable racial attitudes moved toward having even less favorable ones. 3. On the teaching and learning questionnaire, at the beginning of the course and at the end of the course, students with less favorable racial attitudes were significantly more likely than those with more favorable attitudes to believe that ”the main job of the teacher is to transmit the values of the mainstream American culture.” In fact, the significance of this difference increased from the pretest to the posttest 4. Based on their responses to the pre and post administration of the teaching and learning questionnaire, students with more favorable racial attitudes were more likely to change in the desired direction than were students with less favorable racial attitudes. Specifically , although there was no difference in the responses of the two groups to the items below at the beginning of the course, at the conclusion of the course students with less favorable racial attitudes were significantly more likely than those with favorable racial attitudes to still believe that a) tracking in high school courses is desirable. b) all students should be taught in English. c) minority students had had good-excellent opportunities to learn in the high schools which the respondents had attended. d) minority students no longer face unequal opportunities to learn in school. 5. There were no differences between students with more favorable attitudes and those with less favorable attitudes in terms of their attendance patterns, their performance on the midterm and final exams, or their final course grades. Qflitativg an The qualitative data sources were the structured interviews and the unstructured interviews. My analysis of these data resulted in twelve major findings. Throughout this 163 section I refer to racial attitudes as measured by the QDI given to all students at the end of the semester. 1. With only a couple exceptions, students with more favorable racial attitudes and those with less favorable attitudes came from communities and high schools that were very similar in their racial composition, typically 90% or more White. 2. Despite the similarities between the two groups in the racial makeup of their communities and schools, students with more favorable racial attitudes showed greater awareness of racial prejudice and discrimination in their hometowns and high schools than did students with less favorable racial attitudes. 3. Students with more favorable racial attitudes reported more interracial friendships and higher quality interracial relationships than did students with less favorable racial attitudes. 4. Students with more favorable racial attitudes were more likely to believe that racial minorities have unequal opportunities to learn in school. 5. Students with more favorable racial attitudes were more likely to offer situational explanations for the lower scth achievement of minority pupils, while those with less favorable attitudes were more likely to offer personal (or trait) explanations. 6. To address the lower school achievement of minority pupils, students with more favorable racial attitudes were more likely to believe that they would have to do some things differently for their minority pupils, whereas students with less favorable attitudes tended to believe that all students should be treated the same. 7. Although there were no clear differences between the two groups of students in terms of what they reported learning from the course, students with favorable racial attitudes were more likely than those with unfavorable attitudes to report that the comse had led them to increased self -awareness or self -understanding. 8. The two groups of students did not appear to differ in terms of which class sessions they considered most valuable and least valuable. 164 9. The two groups of students did not appear to differ in terms of their overall satisfaction with the course. 10. While the two groups of students reported that being interviewed had little, if any, influence on any of their beliefs, they were nearly unanimous in saying that the interviews had pushed them to think more deeply about what they believed. 11. The data gathered on three focus students indicate that they entered the course with different beliefs about racial minorities, different racial experiences, and different personal qualities, all of which seemed to lead them to react differently to some of the course content 12. The two focus students who displayed more openness to different perspectives and an ability to be self -reflective appeared to be affected more positively by their experience in the course than the student who was less open and less self -reflective. Discussion 1 have talked at length about how students’ racial attitudes and beliefs appeared to mediate what they Ieamed from CH’ 240. In this section I will discuss how the mediation might have occurred and what particular messages students seemed to miss because of this mediation. Also, I will discuss several other things that I Ieamed from conducting this research but which were not reflected in the findings reported earlier. Beliefs and Attitudes as Mediating Eactors Many different researchers (e.g., Bullough, 1991; Feiman-Nemser & Buchmann, 1985; Goodman, 1988; Hollingsworth, 1989; Holt-Reynolds, 1991a; Lottie, 1975; Pajares, 1992; Richardson, 1996) have explained how prospective teachers’ entering beliefs about teaching seem to serve as filters through which the information presented in the teacher education program is interpreted (see Chapter 2). The data collected in this study suggest that, in a similar fashion, information presented to prospective teachers about diversity is filtered through their existing racial attitudes and beliefs. In other words, what students Ieam from a course on diversity is mediated by the racial attitudes 165 and beliefs which they bring to the course. An important question is, how does this mediation occur? My examination of social psychological literature suggests several plausible explanations. Processing of New Information One explanation is that individuals’ existing beliefs and attitudes directly influence how they process new information. Scheibe (1970) and Zimbardo and Leippe (1991) have pointed out that humans tend to respond to new information in a conservative manner and, therefore, are prone to resist having their minds changed, even though they may have no conscious intention or desire to be resistant. In fact, they will often (unwittingly) distort the new information in an effort to make it consistent with their existing beliefs rather than change those beliefs, as Scheibe (1970) explained, preexisting beliefs can distort the interpretation of incoming information so that it is congenial with these beliefs. Incoming information may be distorted (1) to give more support to an entrenched belief than it should or (2) to deny the relevance of i9q§ormation to the belief if the information is sufficiently incompatible with it (p. These two ways of distorting information definitely appeared to be employed by some of the students in my study. During my interviews with Claire, for instance, she expressed numerous beliefs and attitudes that were apparently prejudiced and contradictory to the information being presented in the course. Nevertheless, at the end of the semester she asserted that the course had not led her to any new insights but rather had only reinforced her conviction that she was not prejudiced against anyone. Obviously, she distorted the information presented in the course because otherwise she would have become aware of at least some of the ways in which she displayed prejudiced attitudes and beliefs. Claire also demonstrated the second type of distortion. In one interview I showed her a newspaper article indicating that unemployment rates for Blacks and Hispanics were significantly higher than they were for Whites in the local metropolitan area. Upon seeing the article, Claire responded, I think if I had seen that for, like, ten years ago, I would have said it was because of the problem with not employing somebody because they are Black or because 166 they are Hispanic. As far as now, I have no idea. . . . As far as why there’s more Blacks and Hispanics unemployed, I don’t know, but there’s a variety of reasons for unemployment, and it’s not because of race or color. [Claire 412-427] Quite clearly, Claire rejected the idea that the information I showed her might be an indication of racial discrimination in employment because such an idea was completely incompatible with her existing belief that everyone has equal opportunities. She couldn’t explain the unemployment disparities, yet somehow she was sure that they were not the result of racial discrimination. She resisted accepting the new information and changing her existing belief. I think Claire clearly illustrates the two ways in which people may unwittingly distort information in order to make it more “congenial” with their preexisting beliefs, and it seems quite likely that other students may have engaged in the same types of distortion as Claire. Thus, this distorting of information seems to be one way in which their preexisting attitudes may have mediated what students Ieamed from CEP 240. Stephan (1986) and Zimbardo and Leippe (1991) identified several other ways in which individuals’ existing beliefs and attitudes seem to influence how they process new information. In his review of the literature on intergroup relations, Stephan (1986) reported that existing cognitive structures shape “the processing of information by guiding attention, by structuring encoding, and by determining what information is most likely to be retained, how it will be stored, and the degree to which it will subsequently be available” (p. 604). In short, individuals’ beliefs and attitudes determine how new information is perceived, interpreted, and remembered. Furthermore, Stephan cited research suggesting that individuals’ expectancies (which Scheibe [1970] referred to as “future-oriented beliefs”) bias how they respond to new information. In processing information, people show a tendency to seek out aspects which confirm their expectancies. Thus, in the present study, for example, students who began the course believing (and therefore expecting to hear) that racism and discrimination are no longer serious problems in American society would be likely to focus on and remember most 167 those aspects of the course which seemed to confirm this belief. Zimbardo and Leippe (1991) described a closely related phenomenon called selective attention, which is “a tendency to pay closer attention to a presented message that supports our existing attitude compared to one that opposes it” (p. 144). The authors explained that “despite good intentions to be objective, people tend to gather information in such a way as to stack the deck in favor of confirming their beliefs” (p. 205). By focusing their attention on the aspects of the information that supports their existing beliefs and attitudes, while simultaneously failing to attend to aspects which conflict with them, individuals are able to protect and reaffirm what they already believe and thereby avoid, or at least minimize, cognitive dissonance. Furthermore, Zimbardo and Leippe cited research which suggested that identifying even a few instances of information consistent with individuals’ beliefs seems sufficient to outweigh many more instances of information which is inconsistent, probably because they disregard or fail to attend as closely tothe inconsistent information. This practice of selective attention is probably a partial explanation as to why many of the low students in my study did not change their racial beliefs and attitudes despite all the disconfirrning information presented to them. Finally, Zimbardo and Leippe cited research suggesting that people’s existing beliefs influence not only how they process new information but also how they remember it The authors noted that “‘the past’ is not a fixed, immutable series of events, but often involves subjective interpretation and reconstruction of memories in line with what we believe and value” (p. 207). In my study, Claire provided an excellent illustration of how this type of reconstruction can occur. Early in the semester, shortly after viewing a videotape entitled “True Colors,” which conveyed the message that racial discrimination is still a problem in America, Claire acknowledged that the video illustrated racial discrimination. However, at the end of the semester she described the video as being about diversity, and in none of her comments about it did she make any reference to the idea of discrimination (see Chapter 4). Because the video’s message about the continuing existence of racial 168 discrimination was inconsistent with Claire’s deeply-held belief that everyone has equal opportunities in this society, she apparently reconstructed the message of the video to make it consistent with that belief. Again, it seems very likely that many other students engaged in similar types of reconstructions of the information presented in CEP 240. Consistency Theories A second plausible explanation of the mediation process is found in a broad body of social psychological research generally referred to as consistency theories. While there are many different varieties of consistency theories, Oskamp (1991) has succinctly characterized their basic nature as follows: their key feature is the principle that people try to maintain consistency among their beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors. Awareness of one’s own inconsistency is viewed as an uncomfortable situation which every person is motivated to escape. Thus, attitude change should result if individuals receive new information which is inconsistent with their previous viewpoints or if existing inconsistencies in their beliefs and attitudes are pointed out to them. (pp. 230-231) Although consistency theories view people as essentially rational and logical beings, they also recognize that people are often irrational and illogical. As explained in the previous section, people are inclined to interpret new information in such a way as to make it consistent with what they already believe. When presented with information that is inconsistent with their existing beliefs and attitudes, they will go to great lengths to resolve the inconsistencies and make the information congruent with their existing vieWpoints. Consequently, no matter how much information is proffered nor how compelling the information may be, merely presenting people with information which contradicts their existing beliefs and attitudes provides no guarantee that any change will occur (Zimbardo & Leippe, 1991; Oskamp, 1991). This human tendency toward consistency might be another way in which the existing attitudes and beliefs of the students in CEP 240 mediated what they learned from the course Bern (1970) identified four ways in which individuals strive to maintain cognitive consistency in the face of contradictory evidence: denial, bolstering, differentiation, and transcendence. Though I can’t be certain that participants in my study employed any or 169 all of these devices, it seems highly probable that they did. First, individuals may flatly deny the truth of the inconsistent evidence. Thus, when low students who began CEP 240 believing that everyone has equal opportunities in school were presented with information suggesting that minorities still have unequal opportunities to Ieam, the low students may have simply rejected this information as untrue. Second, when an existing belief or attitude is challenged by contrary evidence, individuals may attempt to bolster the belief or attitude “by seeking out other supportive beliefs and thus attempt to swamp the inconsistency” (p. 28). In so doing, the importance and the impact of the inconsistency is diminished. For example, low students may have recalled personal experiences they had had, articles they had read, or stories they had heard from others about how opportunities have improved for minorities, as evidenced by school segregation being illegal, by the apparently increasing numbers of minorities attending college, by the availability of special scholarships and other financial assistance for minorities, by minorities’ rising scores on SAT tests, and so on. Perhaps these students then reason that in light of all of these apparent positives, the fact that a few minorities might still experience some inequalities may seem less consequential and, therefore, insufficient to change their basic belief that opportunities are equal. Third, individuals may lessen the significance of the inconsistent evidence by differentiating it in some way. For example, they may possibly reason that inequalities faced by minorities are the result of two different causes: through unfair treatment by the majority or through some inadequacies in the victims themselves. They may further reason that inequalities resulting from the latter cause are somehow less consequential. Thus, even in light of information indicating that inequalities exist today, these students are able to view them as not being of the same order as those which existed in the past as a consequence of overt racial discrimination. Finally, evidence conflicting with individuals’ existing beliefs and attitudes may be explained away through transcendence, “a kind of reverse differentiation in that it sees the two disparate beliefs as part of a larger, or transcendent 170 unity” (p. 29). For some of the low students in this study, an example of a larger unity might possibly be that humans, and therefore human institutions, are imperfect. These students may believe that although every effort is being made to ensure equal opportunities for all, inevitably a few inequalities will continue to exist. Thus, in their minds, the existence of a few instances of inequality would not invalidate their idea that opportunities are equal. Belief IAttitude Strength Bern (1970) and Zimbardo and Leippe (1991) have suggested a third plausible explanation of the mediation process: the strength of people’s beliefs and attitudes. Bem (1970) explained that beliefs have a vertical structure (depth) and a horizontal structure (breadth). Depth refers to how well elaborated a particular belief is (i.e., how many reasons or arguments support a single belief), and breadth refers to how extensively a particular belief is connected to and supported by other related beliefs. Rokeach (1976) has described the structure of attitudes in an almost identical fashion, although he used the term difi'erentiation rather than depth to refer to “the degree of articulation of the various parts within a whole” (p. 117). Beliefs and attitudes which have greater depth and breadth will show greater perseverance in the face of contradictory information (Bern, 1970; Rokeach, 1976; Zimbardo & Leippe, 1991). Thus, in the present study, students’ beliefs and attitudes that were already well developed (i.e., having considerable depth and/or breadth) were unlikely to be much affected by the information presented during the course. Another important dimension of belief/attitude strength described by both Bern (1970) and Rokeach (1976) is centrality, which refers to how important or how central a particular belief/attitude is in an individual’s belief/attitude system. They point out that a belief or attitude may have considerable depth and/or breadth but still not be particularly important to the person; on the other hand, it is possible for a belief or attitude that is not well elaborated to be very important in the person’s belief or attitude system. The greater the degree of centrality of a particular belief or attitude, the more 171 resistant it will be to change. Although they use the term involvement rather than centrality, Zimbardo and Leippe (1991) have explained that this dimension leads to greater resistance because people are typically most unbudgeable on issues that involve them personally to the point where their sense of personal identity, accustomed way of life, or sense of social connectedness is tied tightly to their stand on the issue. (p. 204) Zimbardo and Leippe argued that understanding people’s involvement with an issue is a key to understanding why some attitudes are so resistant to change. In my study it seems probable that the centrality of some students’ beliefs and attitudes on (or their involvement with) the issues covered in this course was a conuibuting factor to the apparent lack of change in their attitudes and beliefs. Message As a consequence of the various ways in which students’ racial attitudes and beliefs served to mediate what they learned, many of the students seemed to miss some of the main messages the course intended to convey. Based on my involvement with the course as an instructor of one discussion section, I believe that there were certain key messages relative to racial issues which the CEP 240 course attempted to convey to prospective teachers. In my perception, these messages included the following: 1) racism and discrimination are still major problems in American society, 2) many minority students still do not have equal opportunities to learn in school, 3) a number of current school policies and practices serve to depress the school achievement of many minority students, and 4) in their instruction prospective teachers need to be aware of and prepared to respond to the special instructional needs of many racial minority students. The data reported in the previous chapter seem to indicate that most of the students who scored high on the QDI got the intended messages, while most of those scoring low missed the messages. I would say that a student " got" the messages when, at the end of the course, his/her responses to questionnaire items or to interview questions seemed congruent with the intended messages, and a student "missed” the messages when, at the end of the 172 course, his/her responses to questionnaire items or interview questions seemed incongruent with the intended messages. To say that a message was ”missed” implies that a student did not hear or did not comprehend it; however, I am using the term to also encompass those who may have misinterpreted or outright rejected the intended message. Although some students expressed ideas at the beginning of the semester that were already congruent with the key messages of the course, the data suggest that many students' ideas changed during the semester, probably as a result of their exposure to the course, whereas the ideas of many other students did not appear to change despite their exposure to the course. It is this latter group of students, those who apparently missed the intended messages of the course, with whom I am presently concerned. One of the central messages of the course was that racism and discrimination are still major problems in American society. In response to this item on the QDI at the end of the semester, over 75% of the hi gh-scoring students strongly or mostly agreed that racism and discrimination are still problems, whereas only 31% of the low-scoring students strongly or mostly agreed. This large difference between the two groups of students suggest that most of the low students missed this particular message. A second key message of the course was that many minority students do not have equal opportunities to Ieam in school. Students' responses to item #25 on the ABCD questionnaire would seem to clearly indicate that the students with more favorable racial attitudes got that message, while those with less favorable attitudes missed it At the beginning of the semester 69.6% of the high students and 55.5% of the low students agreed that minority students face unequal opportunities. This difference between the two groups was not significant (p>.315). However, at the conclusion of the course the percentage of high students expressing agreement with the statement increased to 95.6%, while the low students agreeing decreased to 49.2%. This difference between the groups was highly significant (p>.0000). Apparently, a sizable percentage of the high students 173 changed their response to this item while the low students were mostly unchanged. Hence, I would argue that many of the lows missed this message. A third key message of the course was that some current school policies and practices serve to depress the school achievement of many minority students. One specific school practice that received considerable attention in the course was tracking. On the ABCD questionnaire at the beginning of the semester, 36.9% of the highs and 25.9% of the lows expressed disagreement with the practice of tracking students. This difference was not significant (p>.5737). However, at the conclusion of the course 42.8% of the highs and 27.3% of the lows expressed disagreement While neither percentage appeared to change very much, this difference was significant (p>.0293). Therefore, once again it appeared that more lows than highs missed one of the intended messages of the course. A fourth key message of the course was that prospective teachers need to be aware of and prepared to respond to the special instructional needs of many minority students. Although none of the questionnaire items addressed this issue directly, the responses of the fourteen target students to some of the interview questions suggest a difference between highs and lows. Four of the seven low-scoring target students expressed the belief that minority students should receive the same instruction as everyone else, whereas five of the seven hi gh-scoring target students believed they would need to do some things differently for many minority learners. As with the other key messages, more lows than highs seemed to miss this intended message. er 'n s I designed this study to seek answers to the three research questions set forth in Chapter 1; however, actually conducting the study led me to a number of additional Iearnings. 174 Collecting both quantitative and qualitative data led me to an awareness of an important limitation of attitude surveys like the one used in my study. While students’ scores on such instruments may provide a reasonably good indication of the direction of students’ racial attitudes (i.e., positive, neutral, or negative), these scores actually tell us little about the strength of the students’ attitudes, about the beliefs and prior experiences serving as the basis for the attitudes, or about their susceptibility to change. I Ieamed from interviewing students that each of these three variables seemed to be important in understanding the students and their response to the course, and they would certainly seem to be essential considerations in any systematic effort to change the students’ attitudes. Additionally, from my interviews with students I Ieamed that there were clear differences not only between hi gh-scoring students and low-scoring students, but also between students within each of these two groups. The major between-group differences I have detailed in Chapter 4 and summarized earlier in this chapter. Witlrin-group differences became apparent when I analyzed the interview data for the 14 target students. Among the seven low-scoring students that I interviewed, two seemed to be quite set in their attitudes and did not appear to be particularly receptive to the major messages of the course, while two other students seemed far less rigid and appeared more willing to question and re-examine their existing attitudes and beliefs. For example, Claire and Joy scored similarly on their pre and post attitude surveys, but my analysis of their interview data revealed huge differences both in how open they appeared to be to the information presented in the course and in how they interpreted that information (see Chapter 4). The three remaining low-scoring students appeared to fall in-between these extremes. Likewise, among the hi gh-scoring students, the qualitative data revealed some noteworthy differences between students who had similar scores on the attitude survey. There was considerable variation among the students in terms of the number of interracial friendships they reported having, their prior experiences with individuals from different racial backgrounds, and their apparent understanding of racism and discrimination. For 175 example, Teni reported having many friends from different racial groups, had extensive prior interracial experiences, and often displayed extraordinary insight and understanding of racial issues. Although Tina reported fewer interracial friendships and less extensive prior interracial experiences, in many respects she demonstrated insight and understanding comparable to Terri’s. In contrast, Cindy and Ernest each reported few interracial friendships, had only modest prior interracial eXperiences, and did not display the deep understandings that Terri and Tina did. While some of the information presented in the course seemed to lead Cindy and Ernest to new insights and understandings, for Tina and Terri it appeared to primarily reinforce and extend what they already knew. In summary, I have Ieamed the importance of being cautious in how I interpret and use students’ scores on an attitude survey. The scores that students receive tell us little about the actual strength (depth and breadth) of their attitudes, and students receiving similar scores may vary considerably in some important respects. Some students' comments in the interviews suggest that, as their interviewer, I may have unintentionally played a mediating role in the learning of the fourteen target students. Although eleven of the target students asserted that the interviews had had little or no effect on any of their attitudes or beliefs, thirteen of them felt that the interviews had pushed them to think more deeply about the course material. Because the interviews had pushed them to think more deeply, several students, lows and highs alike, expressed the belief that they had gained more from the course. I found this circumstance interesting but somewhat surprising because, as I explained in Chapter 4, I had never intended for the interviews to be an intervention. The purpose of the interviews was to gather information about students’ attitudes and beliefs and about their experience of the course; the purpose was not to increase students’ engagement with and learning from the course; nevertheless, for most of the target students, it appears that the latter is precisely what happened. Having the opportunity to talk with someone about the issues being dealt with in the course seemed to encourage the students to think more about them. In the 176 interviews I generally attempted to reflect back to the students what they were saying to me, but I also attempted to push them to explain what they believed and why they believed it. Being called upon to explain and then justify what they believed seemed to lead students to greater self -awareness and self -understanding. As one student explained, ‘you made me, like, realize a lot of things about myself that I probably wouldn’t have realized unless I would have talked to you” (Joy 428-4). From my experience in this study, I have learned that another way to increase the potential impact of a course on diversity may be to provide regular opportunities for students to discuss the issues and to examine and justify their beliefs about them. From the interviews I also learned that students' prior interracial experiences seemed to be of major importance in shaping their racial attitudes and beliefs. First of all, I found that high and low students alike all seemed to rely heavily on their personal experiences for justification of their beliefs about various issues. More often than not, when I asked students to explain the basis for a particular belief, if they were able to offer an explanation at all, they almost invariably cited some type of personal experience or the experience of someone they knew. This strong tendency that I observed seems very much consistent with Bem’s (1970) assertion that “every belief can be pushed back until it is seen to rest ultimately upon a basic belief in the credibility of one’s own sensory experience or upon a basic belief in the credibility of some external authority” (p. 5). As I explained in Chapter 4, I found some evidence that when information presented in the course conflicted with students’ experience-based beliefs, they often seemed to give more credence to their own experience than to the conflicting information. Secondly, given the apparent importance of prior experiences in shaping attitudes and beliefs, it was not surprising to me that students whose experiences with diversity had been very limited generally demonstrated more limited beliefs about diversity issues. For example, ~ although they tended to grow up in communities that were very similar to those of hi gh- scoring students in terms of their racial composition, low-scoring target students reported 177 considerably fewer interracial friendships, demonstrated much less awareness of racial prejudice and discrimination in their schools and communities, and generally expressed less favorable attitudes toward and beliefs about racial minorities (see Chapter 4). These findings suggest to me that there may be a relationship between students’ prior interracial experiences and their racial attitudes and beliefs. However, my study provides insufficient data to justify speculating much about the nature of this possible relationship. This is a question that needs to be investigated more directly in future research. Not withstanding the need for further research, the data collected in this study suggests to me that instructors of diversity courses must give attention to students’ prior interracial experiences (or lack thereof). Perhaps, too, in addition to simply presenting students with information, instructors should also seek opportunities to somehow broaden the experiences of those who come with limited prior interracial experiences. One additional personal insight that I gained from this study may possibly be related to the important role that personal experience seems to play in shaping people’s attitudes and beliefs. In examining the class sessions that students considered the most and least valuable, I could determine no noteworthy differences between high and low students in terms of the sessions that they identified. When I looked at the class sessions that both groups of students had considered least valuable, I could not point out any strong similarities. However, when I looked at the class sessions that the two groups had considered most valuable, there did appear to be an interesting similarity in their responses. I found that the class sessions that both high and low students considered most valuable tended to be either the showing of a videotape or a presentation by a panel. A number of comments from students suggest that these types of experiences had a greater impact on them than readings or lectures. For example, Claire, a low student, explained how the F.A.T. City video had enhanced her understanding of Iearrring disabled children. I really liked that one, putting it on a normal everyday level for like you and I. We saw what a, perhaps, learning disability child might be going through and a 178 lot of those things you never stop to really think about ‘em. . . . I think that it made me see what kinds of problems that they go through. You know, I mean it’s easy to say, yeah, okay, they might be slow. Yeah, they might turn letters around or something like that, but this actually, you know, put it in terms that we could understand it. [Claire 429-26] Karen, one of the high students, responded to the video in a similar fashion. Just watching that made you understand what the kid goes through. You know, (inaudible) sitting in a classroom. I mean, that kinda made you understand how they feel. You’ll take that with you when you teach. [Karen 427-24] Students’ responses to the videos suggest that they can be a valuable tool in helping students come to understand people who are different than themselves. They can provide students with a type of vicarious experience with people and situations they may not have encountered yet in real life. Students’ comments indicated that panel presentations also seemed to facilitate their understanding of others. One the low students, Kate, expressed a change in attitude toward gays and lesbians as a result of the bi/gay/lesbian panel presentation. I asked Kate to explain why. Just ‘cause I never got to like hear from one of them. I never really, I mean, I’ve seen like one or two maybe, but I’ve never seen like a panel discussing about how they’re like in love with a partner and like things like that I don’t know. It just helped a little. [It] helped me understand them. [Kate 421-70] One of the high students, Cindy, expressed a similar change in attitude as a consequence of hearing the panel and actually meeting a person who was homosexual. I’ve never had a friend who is homosexual. And like we had that panel and then I got to know somebody and it just helped me to accept them more and not look at it as totally wrong. I’ve never thought it was totally wrong, but it just helped me, I think, to be more accepting. To, you know, to hear about their own personal stories and everything. [Cindy 506-26] Comments like these suggest to me that for some students--particularly those whose prior experiences have been limited»providing them with opportunities to meet and interact with people from different groups may be a promising way to promote positive attitude change. Conclusions The major findings in this study lead me to draw several conclusions. First, White preservice teachers bring to their multicultural teacher education courses different 179 attitudes toward racial minority groups. While some of them come with mostly positive attitudes, others--approximately one-third of the sample in this study--come with less positive perceptions of minorities, little awareness of racism and discrimination, less favorable views on multicultural education, and less positively disposed toward teaching minority group children. Other research (Bymes & Ki ger, 1988; Law & Lane, 1987) has also found that White preservice teachers' attitudes toward racial minorities are not always positive, and additional research (Duckitt, 1992; Maluso, 1995) has shown that people's racial attitudes influence how they behave toward members of another racial group. Given the fact that the student population in our schools is expected to become increasingly diverse in the coming years, less than favorable racial attitudes on the part of many prospective teachers are certainly a cause for concern, for a number of reasons. First, the data collected in this study indicate that prospective teachers who hold less favorable attitudes toward racial minorities are more likely to attribute the lower school achievement of minority students primarily to personal (or trait) factors rather than to situational factors. With this perspective, these prospective teachers are more likely than those with more favorable racial attitudes to “blame the victims” for their lower achievement, and they may also be inclined to hold lower expectations for these students. Second, the data in this study indicate that prospective teachers who hold less favorable racial attitudes are more likely than those who hold more favorable attitudes to believe that all students should be treated the same. This suggests that these teachers are likely to be less willing than those with more favorable attitudes to make appropriate instructional adjustments to accommodate the different needs of minority pupils in their classrooms. Finally, prospective teachers with less favorable racial attitudes report having less desire to work with minority children. Individuals who have indicated they would prefer not to work with particular students because of their racial backgrounds would seem to be more likely to behave in a negative fashion toward those students. 180 A second conclusion is that prospective teachers' racial attitudes and beliefs appeared to serve as filters through which they interpreted the information and experiences that they were presented within the course. The data indicate that the participants in this study often tended to interpret the inf orrnation presented to them in a manner consistent with their existing racial attitudes and beliefs. The idea that prospective teachers entering attitudes and beliefs serve as filters for processing the content of their teacher education program has been frequently mentioned in the teacher education literature (Borko & Putnam, in press; Goodman, 1988; Hollingsworth, 1989; Kagan, 1992; Richardson, 1996). Furthermore, the fact that individuals' attitudes and beliefs influence the acquisition and interpretation of new knowledge has also been well documented in the social psychological research (Bar-Ta], 1990: Bem, 1970; Oskamp, 1991; Petty & Cacioppo, 1981; Zimbardo & Leippe, 1991). Thus, the conclusion that prospective teachers' racial attitudes and beliefs serve as filters for their learning of information on diversity is very much consistent with current research in both areas. Third, the racial attitudes and beliefs of preservice teachers appeared to be little affected by the course. For the most part, students' entering racial attitudes and beliefs, whether more favorable or less favorable, appeared to be reinforced by the course rather than changed. Research results on the impact of diversity courses on students' attitudes and beliefs have been mixed. Some researchers (Bondy, Schmitz, & Johnson, 1993; McGeehan, 1982; Reed, 1993; Ross & Smith, 1992; Tran, Young, & Di Lelia, 1994) have reported that students' racial attitudes and beliefs have been changed in a positive direction by a course on diversity, but others (Barry & Lechner, 1995; Larke, 1990; McDiarmid, 1992; McDiarmid & Price, 1990) have reported little or no change in students' attitudes and beliefs. One distinctive feature of the present research which sets it apart from earlier research in this area was that change among students with more favorable racial attitudes was examined separately from change among students with less favorable racial attitudes. If, as the results of this study suggest, favorable racial attitudes 181 tend to become more favorable and unfavorable attitudes tend to become less favorable during the course, it may be inappropriate to attempt to measure attitude change among a class of students as a whole because differences of this type existing within the class would be obscured. Fourth, students' prior equal-status contact with racial minorities seems to be related to having more favorable racial attitudes and beliefs. One of the most striking differences between students with more favorable racial attitudes and those with less favorable attitudes was the reported differences in their previous interracial relationships. Despite coming from communities that were, for the most part, very similar in their racial composition, students with more favorable racial attitudes reported in both the survey data and the interview data that they had greater quantity and higher quality relationships with individuals from other racial groups. Other researchers (McGeehan, 1982; Ross & Smith, 1992) have reported similar findings. Although it is possible that having more positive racial attitudes is what leads to more interracial relationships, research cited by Maluso (1995) suggests that it is the equal-status interracial contact which leads to more positive interracial attitudes. Fifth, there did not appear to be any relationship between students' racial attitudes and what they learned from the course or how they felt about the course. There was no difference in the performance of the two groups of students on the midterm and final exams, both of which were primarily assessments of factual knowledge. In addition, during the final interview when the target students were asked to identify the most important things that they had Ieamed from the course, I could discern no apparent differences between the responses of students with more favorable racial attitudes and those with less favorable racial attitudes. This finding stands in contrast to that of McGeehan (1982), who found that students with the most positive interethnic attitudes were the ones demonstrating the most knowledge growth. Also, in my final interviews with the target students, there appeared to be no difference between those with more 182 favorable attitudes and those with less favorable attitudes in terms of their level of satisfaction with the course. All of the target students expressed very positive feelings about the course. McGeehan found that some students responded more positively than others to the course on diversity, but she could find no pattern of common characteristics for those who responded more positively and those who responded less positively. I think these findings indicate that it may be inappropriate to assume that students with less favorable racial attitudes will respond less favorably to a course on diversity than students with more favorable racial attitudes. They further suggest that students' positive feelings about a course on diversity does not necessarily mean that their attitudes were positively influenced by the course. Sixth, contrary to the findings of McGeehan's (1982) study, the amount of factual information that students Ieam from a course in diversity does not appear to be related to their racial attitudes; however, the meanings that students attach to this information do seem related to their racial attitudes. In other words, though the students in the course were all exposed to the same information, those holding less favorable racial attitudes appeared to construct different meanings for the information than those holding more positive racial attitudes. The data collected in this study suggest that in response to the information presented in CEP 240, students holding more favorable racial attitudes, in contrast to those holding less favorable attitudes, were much more likely to move toward embracing viewpoints that are generally considered to be more sensitive to the needs of diverse learners. The idea that prospective teachers' entering attitudes and beliefs serve as filters for subsequent learning has been well documented in the literature (Anderson & Holt-Reynolds, 1995; Feiman-Nemser & Buchmann, 1989; Goodman, 1988; Hollingsworth, 1989; Holt-Reynolds, 1991b; Kagan, 1992; Richardson, 1996). Finally, there is some evidence that multicultural education courses like the one investigated in this study may be most effective for students who already possess more favorable racial attitudes and for those who display a quality of openness and an ability to 183 be self -reflective. There appeared to be no difference between the two groups of students in terms of their knowledge growth from the course (as indicated by their scores on the midterm and final exams), but as noted earlier, the students with more favorable racial attitudes were the ones who seemed to internalize more of the central messages of the class, particularly the message that many minority students still face unequal opportunities to learn. Additionally, my analysis of the data on Joy and Claire, the two case study students who were identified as having less favorable racial attitudes, indicated that Joy's attitudes and beliefs appeared to be moving in the desired direction in some respects because of her openness to different perspectives and because of her inclination to be self -reflective, whereas Claire's attitudes and beliefs appeared to be largely unaffected by the course because she seemed reluctant to look at issues from a different perspective and was not very self -reflective. While the data collected in this study clearly support the idea that students' entering racial attitudes and beliefs serve as filters through which they perceive the information being presented to them, the differences that I observed in how Joy and Claire (both of whom scored low on the QDI) responded to the course would seem to suggest that a student's openness to new information and his/her ability to be self -reflective may be equally important factors affecting how they perceive information on diversity. It further suggests that some students who enter a diversity . course with less favorable racial attitudes are more likely than others with similar attitudes to be moved in the desired direction by the course. Limitations of the Study There are several key limitations that need to be considered in interpreting the results of this study. One important limitation was that the Quick Discrimination Index (QDI), the racial attitude survey, was administered only to the volunteer students at the beginning of the course and again at the end of the course. The rest of the class completed the survey only at the end of the course. Ideally, all students should have completed the QDI pre and post. I dealt with this limitation by establishing that the 184 students who volunteered were not significantly different from those who did not volunteer. I could then reasonably infer from the performance of the target students how the students in the class as a whole would have performed on a pre-administration of the QDI. A second limitation of this study is that the principal investigator was Black and all of the participants were White. Research cited by Maluso (1995) suggests that White respondents are not always completely honest about their feelings when addressing a Black interviewer. I addressed this limitation by asking each of the target students whether or not their responses to my question had ever been influenced by the fact that they were being interviewed by a Black person. The target students were unanimous in saying that my being Black had little or no influence on how they had responded to my questions; however, I still must acknowledge that sometimes their responses may well have been influenced in ways in which they were not consciously aware. A third limitation of this study is that many students, in response to the items on the racial attitude survey and on the teaching and leaming questionnaire, may have provided responses that were socially desirable rather than responses that indicated their true attitudes and beliefs. I could argue, however, that it is probable that this effect was randomly distributed across the entire sample and, in that respect, was its own control. Another possibility is that students with less favorable attitudes might be the ones more inclined to give socially desirable responses rather than their true views. However, even this occurrence would not necessarily invalidate the results I reported; rather, it might suggest that the differences between students with more favorable racial attitudes and beliefs and those with less favorable attitudes and beliefs may actually be even greater than those which I observed. Implications The findings from this study, taken together, would seem to have a number of implications for teacher educators. First, there appears to be a clear connection between 185 prospective teachers' attitudes toward racial minorities and their openness to the content of a multicultural education course. The data suggest that teacher candidates who enter a multicultural teacher education course with less favorable attitudes toward minorities are significantly less likely to alter their entering beliefs about minority students than are candidates who enter with more favorable attitudes. It appears that, even after exposure to a multicultural education course, teacher candidates who hold less favorable attitudes toward minorities will be less likely to embrace teaching practices generally believed to be desirable for effective teaching of minority students. Specifically, these teacher candidates are more likely to support tracking of students, be less tolerant of language diversity, and be less willing to accommodate individual differences in their instruction. The findings of this study suggest that preparation programs will need to either restructure their courses on diversity to better take into account the racial attitudes of prospective teachers or provide more than a single multicultural education course of the nature described in this study if they hope to change these teacher candidates' beliefs and attitudes. In a course like CEP 240, for example, which attempts to present information on such a wide range of diversity issues, not just on racial issues, I think it is impossible to also have sufficient time to identify and attempt to address students' attitudes on each of the issues. Although CEP 240 was well-liked by the students and appeared to have a positive impact on many of them, based on the research cited earlier in this report, I believe that a course with an attitude change focus would necessarily have to be structured and taught differently than CEP 240 or most other multicultural education courses which primarily focus on the presentation of information about diversity. Specifically, I believe that, to be effective, multicultural courses will need to be reconstructed so as to acknowledge and then confront unfavorable racial attitudes that prospective teachers bring with them to their preparation programs. Although I doubt that the alternative option»offering a second course that is of the same nature as present courses--would lead to the desired changes in prospective teachers who enter with less 186 favorable racial attitudes and beliefs, it is nonetheless possible that more students might respond favorably to messages on diversity if they hear these messages for a longer time and in different ways. A second implication is that since students' racial attitudes seem to serve as filters through which they interpret the inf orrnation presented in a course on diversity, the entering beliefs and attitudes of students will need to be identified and taken into consideration in the instruction. This means that different students may need to be taught different things and in different ways in order to challenge unfavorable racial attitudes and beliefs. Once again, achieving this objective will necessitate that courses on diversity be reconceptualized and restructured. I believe that until such reconceptualization and restructuring occurs, it is likely that the racial attitudes and beliefs of prospective teachers will, in most instances, continue to be little affected by their exposure to multicultural teacher education courses. Third, in this study one of the most striking differences that emerged between target students with more favorable attitudes and those with less favorable attitudes was in terms of their previous interracial experiences. The former group reported much more prior contact and closer associations with people different from themselves, whereas the latter group were characterized by very limited exposure to and only marginal associations with nonmajority individuals. The implication is that, although teacher education programs can do little, if anything, to alter the interracial experiences that prospective teachers have prior to the time they begin their preparation programs, these programs can and must do more to promote positive, equal-status contact between their White students and students of color. For one thing, they can redouble their efforts to recruit more minority teacher candidates so that White students will at least have some equal-status interracial contact during their program, presenting the possibility of positive interracial friendships developing. Additionally, considering the lack of students of color in most teacher preparation programs, creative ways might possibly be found to promote 187 interactions between White prospective teachers and students of color in other program areas. Certainly, teacher education programs cannot be expected to become social coordinators; however, because research (Duckitt, 1992; Maluso, 1995; Ponterotto & Pedersen, 1993) suggests that equal-status contact with members of different racial groups is one of the most effective ways of reducing racial stereotyping and prejudice, teacher education programs would be remiss to not put forth some effort in this regard. Another way to promote positive interracial contact for White prospective teachers may be to recruit more professors of color into teacher preparation programs. Research by Maluso (1995) has suggested that White racial attitudes improve when they are exposed to a racial minority person in a hi gher-status, leadership position. Maluso cautioned, however, that the improved racial attitudes did not seem to generalize necessarily to members of other racial groups. Presently, it seems that efforts to promote interracial contact for White prospective teachers have mostly been limited to providing them with one or more field experiences in diverse settings. As noted earlier, research suggests that this type of unequal-status contact is less effective, if not completely counterproductive, than equal-status contact I believe the practice of providing prospective teachers with field experiences in multicultural settings should continue, but by no means should doing so be regarded as a substitute for promoting equal-status and higher-status interracial contacts for teacher candidates. Though apparently referring to inservice teachers, Grant (1994) observed that "little work has been done on effective techniques that can be used to change teachers' racial attitudes and behavior“ (p. 251), but he went on to note that Courses that consist primarily or exclusively of lecture presentations have little impact Diverse experiences, such as seminars, visitations, community involvement, committee work, guest speakers, films, multimedia materials, and workshops, combined with factual lectures, are more effective than is any single approach. Community involvement and cross-cultural interactions (with the appropriate norms in the social setting) are the most cogent techniques. (p. 252) Fourth, the presentation of factual inf orrnation as the primary goal of courses on diversity and the attendant tendency to use how much factual information students learn as the primary measure of a course’s effectiveness may be inappropriate. The students in 188 the CEP 240 course were all exposed to the same information, but those with more favorable attitudes t0ward minorities appeared to interpret and utilize this information differently than those with less favorable attitudes. Apparently, effectively preparing students for diversity involves more than just filling their heads with information; an effective intervention may also have to provide them with guidance and support in processing the information and in integrating it with their existing beliefs. Thus, it may be more appropriate to determine the effectiveness of a multicultural education course by assessing its overall impact (or lack thereof) on students’ racial attitudes and beliefs. Fifth, since the mere presentation of factual information does not appear to be associated with changes in students’ racial attitudes and beliefs, perhaps multicultural courses should attempt to cover less information but in more depth so that students will have more time to really think about the information being presented to them. Several of the target students commented in their final interviews that, for them, the biweekly interviews had been an invaluable part of the semester because my questioning had forced them to think more deeply about what they believed and about what they were learning in the course. This study suggests that multicultural courses should probably be structured in such a way as to provide students with regular opportunities for reflection on what they are learning, something which could be accomplished in a number of different ways. Obviously, most professors are not able to have individual conversations with their students; however, there would be more opportunities for these kinds of dialogues between professors and students if class sizes were smaller. Also, it has been my experience that some of this interaction can occur successfully through written mediums, like dialogue journals and e-mail. Additionally, students could be involved in more small group discussions where they would have the opportunity to talk with one another about the information being presented and about their own beliefs. Finally, the findings presented in this study raise the question of whether the selection of candidates for admission to teacher education programs should be based in 189 part on their entering racial attitudes and experiences, as Haberman (1987) has suggested. Gomez (1994) has also argued that teacher educators may need to select their teacher candidates more carefully from the pool of available applicants. Available evidence suggests that multicultural education courses may be most effective for those who already possess favorable racial attitudes and least effective for those who possess unfavorable attitudes. If teacher education programs are unable to change the racial attitudes and beliefs that prospective teachers bring with them into their preparation programs, it may make sense to select only those candidates who bring the desired attitudes and beliefs. Future Research There is still a lot of research needed in the area of multicultural teacher education. First, I think the present study needs to be replicated to test the validity of the major findings reported here. We need to know to what extent the pattern of results obtained in this study seem to hold true with other participants in other course configurations in other settings. We also need to study the effects of diversity courses on students from various minority groups. Second, we need more research that examines the differential effects that courses in diversity may have for students who enter the course with different racial attitudes and beliefs. Looking at students' attitudes only as a whole class may mask important differences in the ways that students with different racial attitudes and beliefs are responding to the course. To better understand students' , responses to attitude surveys, these data sources need to be supplemented with interview data. Third, we need to study courses and/or programs that seem to have been effective in changing prospective teachers' racial attitudes and beliefs to determine which particular aspects of these courses or programs appear to be most critical to their success. If we are to improve the effectiveness of multicultural teacher education courses, we must expand our knowledge of what seems to work and why. Fourth, how students' personal qualities like openness and reflectiveness influence their learning needs to be further explored. Finally, we need some longitudinal research that examines the long-term impact of 190 courses on diversity. Some of the data from this study suggests that the full impact of courses on diversity may not become apparent until months, maybe even years, after the conclusion of the course. APPENDICES APPENDIX A CEP 240 SYLLABUS, READING LIST, AND SAMPLE ACTIVITIES 191 CEP 240 DIVERSE LEARNERS IN MULTICULTURAL PERSPECTIVE SPRING SEMESTER, 1994 Thursday, 8:00-9:50 am, Room B-104 Wells Hall Labs: Section 1--Tuesday, 8950, Room 115 BH (Mary Scott) Section 2--Tuesday, 8-9:50, Room 005 NR (Whitney Hosmer) Section 3--Tuesday, 1920-1210, Room C111 WH (Gretchen Halenbeck) Section 4--Tuesday, 10:20-12: 10, Room 217 Anthony (Mary Scott) Section 5»Tuesday, 12:40-2:30, Room C105 WH (Gretchen Halenbeck) Section 6--Tuesday, 12:40-2:30, Room C213 WH (Anne Mungai) Section 7»Tuesday, 10.20-12:10, Room 317 EBH (Art Garmon) Section 8--Tuesday, 8-9:50, Room C107 WH (Ann Mungai) Instructors: Dr. Eugene Pemell, Jr. Dr. Stanley Trent Offices: Room 338 Erickson Hall Room 342 Erickson Hall Phone: 355-1835 Phone: 355-1835 Office Hours: W 9-11; F 2—4 Office Hours: By appointment (call & schedule apprnt during office hours) Required Texts: Hardman, Michael L., Lif ford, J. Drew, and M. Winston Egan Human Exceptionality: Society Schools and Family. London: Allyn & Bacon, Inc. (1993). Cushner, Kenneth, Averil McClelland and Philip Saf ford. Human Diversity in Education: An Integrative Approach. New Yorlc McGraw-Hill, Inc. (1992). Course Packet -- available at International Bookstore COURSE DESCRIPTION This course aims to provide students with a basic orientation to the nature and pattern of diversity in learners encountered within the context of multi-cultural classrooms. The notion of diversity embraces the full spectrum of differences, including those which place students at risk and those which do not. Further, at-risk factors will incorporate both socio-cultural characteristics and areas of functioning or dysfunctioning that limit access to knowledge. Specifically, such factors include communication, linguistic, gender, cultural, ethnic, physical, sensory, behavioral, affective, and cognitive differences. This view of diversity embraces both individual characteristics and those that exist within and across social-cultural groups, so that students will gain an appreciation of the fact that learners who have a particular characteristic are not a homogeneous group. Student characteristics are studied as culttually mediated and socially constructed identities. The course focuses on diverse learners and the factors that influence their access to learning»both internal to and external to schools. Such factors include in-class assessment and intervention strategies, socialization practices, child rearing styles, linguistic patterns of communication, and other cultural factors. Students will examine the relationships between these cultural and social practices and learner characteristics. 192 The objectives are to I) examine the nature of diversity itself, 2) foster sensitivity and respect for diverse learners, 3) analyze the role of cultural factors that mediate the perceptions and treatment of diversity, 4) enable students to recognize similarities and differences in patterns of development, communication, and learning in multi-cultural settings for students who present diverse characteristics. Cutting across all objectives is the question, How does culture act as a filter to impede or facilitate the effectiveness of instructional strategies when working with diverse learners? In addition to lectures based on material in the required readings, a series of guest speakers will present material in their areas of expertise. The course content is organized into three main blocks: 1 . Who are the diverse students? The nomenclature and definitions of diversity must be studied for a common basis of understanding. The many dimensions of diversity are conceptualized: gender, ethnicity, ability, socio-economic status, etc. 2 . Nature of Diversity In its Multl-cultural context Two macro variables are studied within the context of multi-cultural settings: 1) areas of individual functioning as determined by linguistic, physical and other competencies and 2) the impact of socio-cultural backgrounds such as ethnicity, nationality, socio- economic status, and gender. Thus, diverse learners can be differentiated in terms of individual areas of functioning and in terms of social-cultural backgrounds within an area of functioning. The aim is to examine the ways in which the nature of diversity affects access to learning. 3 . Practical Implications of Learner Diversity In Schooling Practical applications of the study of diverse learner characteristics will be structured around two macro variables: 1) the role of schooling in society--including aspects of socialization, economic livelihood, community life, awareness of the world, appreciation of aesthetic concerns, and 2) the operations of schooling as impacted by learners with diverse characteristics. This focus on the implications of diversity for schooling examines the principles that undergird schooling, its function in society, and the ways schooling is operationalized that are the consequence of responses to diverse learners. The focus is on broad schooling issues and not specific pedagogical strategies. Thus, school policies, classroom structures, legal issues, economics of schooling, preparation of teachers, social relations of schooling with the wider engirqnment, are studied for an understanding of the implications of diversity for so 00 mg. W Students will be expected to complete reading assignments prior to class sessions and to engage in thoughtful participation in lab discussions in light of these assignments. In addition, students will take an in-class midterm and an in-class final exam (objective short answer and multiple choice). Course grades will be based upon a formula: 10% attendance and participation (both lecture and lab sections), 10% lab group assignments, 10% written assignment, 30% mid-tenn, and 40 % final exam. 193 Lab Sections: Students must attend the lab to which they have been assigned. This is essential because activities are planned based on numbers of students and because group continuity is important to the learning process. Explanation of written assiggment: A 3 page paper (typewritten, double-spaced) is due April 19. The subject of the paper is to be a summary and analysis of a children's book that focuses on the subject of disability. Not more than 1 page should be devoted to summary of the story. The analysis should incorporate knowledge gained from class readings, lectures, and lab activities, including but not limited to: l. the extent to which the diversity characteristic is dealt with realistically; 2. treatment of social and/or educational issues surrounding the character and his/her responses to the environment; 3. discussion of any myths, stereotypes, or prejudices that the story (in your opinion) either perpetuates or helps to break down. [Be sure to give evidence to support your conclusions] 4. suggestions for alternative ways of dealing with the subject matter or reasons why the subject is dealt with adequately. A list of children's books which focus on the issues of diversity is provided in your packet, however, you are not limited to this list CEP 240 - COURSE OUTLINE AND SYLLABUS 1/13/94 Orientation to Course, Introductions, Assignments Human Diversity. A Problem And An Opportunity Readings: Cushner, Chapter 1 Hardman, Chapter 1 1/18/94 LAB 1: WHO ARE THE MINORITIES 1/20/94 The History of Human Diversity Readingg: Cushner, Chapter 6 Hardman, Chapter 2 Trent & Artiles, Perceived Changes . . . (Packet) 1/25/94 LAB 2: DISCUSSION: ROLE PLAYING 1/27/94 The Culture Learning Process “Guest Speaker: Cyrus Stewart--recruit for Hawaii Readings: Cushner, Chapter 2 2/1/94 LAB 3: TRUE COLORS 213/94 Analyzing Intercultural Interaction Readings: Cushner, Chapter 3 Hardman, Chapter 7 Lake, An Indian Father’s Plea 2/8/94 LAB 4: MASKS 194 THE NATURE OF DIVERSITY IN A MULTI-CULTURAL CONTEXT 2/10/94 2/15/94 2/17/95 2/22/ 94 2/24/94 Communication Across Culture “Guest Speaker: Dr. David Stewart, Assoc. Professor, CEPSE Readings: Cushner, Chapter 4 Hardman, Chapters 10 & 11 LAB 5: WHAT DO YOU DO WHEN YOU SEEA BLIND MAN Influences on Learning Readings: Cushner, Chapter 5 Hardman, Chapter 6 LAB 6: HOW HARD COULD THIS BE? Multicultural and Bilingual Education "Guest Speaker: Dr. Margie Gallego, Asst. Professor, Dept. of TE Readings: Cushner, Chapter7 Hardman, Chapter 8 3/1/94 LAB 7: VIGNE’I’I‘E' YOLANDA PIEDRA (Packet) 3/3/94 Exceptionality in the Context of Diversity Presentation: The Zimbabwean Experience Readings Cushner, Chapter 8 Hardman, Chapter 12 MID TERM EXAMINATION 3/3/94 3/15/94 LAB 8: REGULAR LIVES 3/17/94 The Experience of Gender: What It Means to Be Male and Female in Society “Guest Panel - Alliance of Lesbian-Bi-Gay Students Readings: Cushner, Chapter 9 Weisstein, “Woman as Nigger” (Packet) 3/22/94 LAB 9 SEX ROLE ST EREOTY PING BACTICAL IMPLICATIONS OF LEARNER DIVERSITY IN SCHOOLING 3/24/94 3/29/94 3/31/94 Expanding Educational Boundaries (Educating Peter) “Guest Speaker: Don Hoyle, President, Wastenaw Association for Community Advocacy Readings: Cushner, Chapter 10 Hardman, Chapters 5 & 9 USNWR - Violence in Schools (Packet) USNWR - Tragedy in Room 1(B (Packet) LAB 10: COOPERATIVE LEARNING The Emerging Educational Community "Community Panel Readings: Cushner, Chapter 11 Hardman, Chapter 14 4/5/94 4/7/94 4112/94 4/14/94 4/19/94 4/21/94 4/26/94 4/28/94 SIS/94 I95 LAB ll: VIGNEI'IE: VANESSA MATTISON (Packet) Transforming the Curriculum and Behavioral Management Readings: Cushner, Chapter 12 Hardman, Chapter 4 Review: Hardman Chapters 5 & 6 LAB 12: BEHAVIOR MANAGEMENT STRATEGIES Transforming Pedagogy "Guest Speaker: Dr. Patricia Edwards, Associate Professor Department of Teacher Education, MSU seams Cushner, Chapter 13 Hardman, Chapter 13 Edwards, Before and After Desegregation (Packet) Edwards, et al., Designing a Collaborative Model . . . LAB 13: RISK TAKERS Transformation of School Culture Readings: Cushner, Chapter 14 Hardman, Chapter 13 (Review) LAB 14: HATS SIMULATION Study Groups for Final Examination FINAL EXAMINATION - 7:45-9:45 a.m. 196 CEP 240 Reading List Edwards, RA. (1993). Before and after school desegregation: African-American parents’ involvement in schools. Educational Policy, 7, 340-369. Lake, R (1990). An Indian father’s plea. Teacher Magazine McIntyre, L.D., & Pemell, E. (19xx). The impact of race on teacher recommendations for special education placement Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development Nieto, S. (1992). Afiirming diversity (pp. 60-68, 181-188). Longman. Tragedy in Room 108. U.S. News & World Report, November 8, 1993, 41-46. Trent, S. C. & Artiles, A. J. (1992) Perceived changes in college students who participated in a multicultural education seminar. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA, April 1992. Violence in schools. U.S. News & World Report, November 8, 1993, 31-36. Weisstein, N. ( 1969). “Woman as Nigger” Psychology Today Whose America? Time Magazine, July 8, I 991 , 12-17. 197 SAMPLE ROLE-PLAYING ACTIVITY Directions: 1. Assign roles to group members by draw. 2. The key role-player whose card asks him/her to identify him/herself begins the discussion. Each group member should have an opportunity to play out their role. (About 15 minutes total). 3. The Recorder/Observer comments and gives feedback to the group. (About 5 minutes) 4. Each member shares their reactions regarding the role they played. (About 10 minutes) 5. After each small group has finished, discuss reactions/concerns as a whole group. BROWN VS. BOARD OF EDUCATION The Supreme Court has just ruled that all schools must be desegregated (black/white). It is the first day of classes in your high school, which Ins been integrated according to court mandate. To clear the air, the teacher has asked you to speak openly about your feelings regarding integration. TEACHER (Identify yourself to the mp). You are white, with private reservations about whether integration will work. WHITE STUDflI #1 Your family has taught you that blacks are lazy and no good. WHITE STUDENT #2 You believe black students will add to the school, especially in the sports program. B CK STUD #1 Your mother is a Baptist minister. She has taught you to treat everyone with respect and that integration is a right of all students. B CK S ENI‘ #2 Your experience has taught you not to trust any White person. You are leery of them and their hidden motives. OBSfliVER/RECORDER (Identify yourself to the my) Watch your group members for their reactions (both verbal and body language). At the end of the roleplaying, tell them what you saw and how you interpret it 198 SAMPLE WHOLE CLASS ACTIVITY WHO ARE THE MINORITIES? Goal: To promote awareness in the class members that we are all minorities in some ways Directions Each student will be given a sheet of paper and the following instructions: Please write your ethnic origin(s) and begin a story about yourself, i.e.,-- Once upon a time my parents gave birth to a healthy baby. The mother’s ethnic origin(s) is German/Polish/Russian, and the father’s ethnic orgin is Irish/Polish. This child sees her/himself as Polish/Russian...etc. He is now 22 years old, enrolled in a big university where he... You will be asked to £99 and get up and move around and find someone whose name or features or information will help you to determine if they are “like” you. They must have at least two similar ethnic origins. Exchange papers with that individual to continue writing each other’s story until the directive “Stop” is given. You will be allowed two minutes to write an ending to the story. ghen the stories are finished, each set of partners should construct and discuss the list low: Ethnic origin(s) Racial identity Religion Number of siblings Favorite sport Favorite ethnic foods Age or age group Last experience of helping someone Desired profession 10. Acquainted with someone of a difference race PWHP‘MPPN!‘ As a class, discuss students’ reactions to the activity. 199 SAMPLE SMALL GROUP ACTIVITY BEHAVIOR MANAGEMENT EXERCISE Allow me to introduce you to Joey, who is an exceptional student. Because I have worked with him, frustratingly at times, I thought that you would be interested in how I see him and use information from class presentations to plan ways to ameliorate his disabilities. He is a boy who is essentially normal; that is, his hearing is normal, his vision is normal, and his intelligence is well within normal range. Yet he does not learn well, and what he learns is labored and often soon forgotten. But, Joey is “normal.” He is 10 years old assigned to the 4th grade. He has visual, auditory, motor, and behavior difficulties. Symptoms are listed below. [I am omitting this half-page list of symptoms from my description of this activity] . . . . What is important is that I have to live and work with this boy six hours a day, five days a week, knowing that in his present learning state he has every guarantee of being a 16-year-old drop out Joey is not in a self contained classroom with a maximum of eight children and he does not have access to a resource room with a specially trained teacher. Joey’s background provided us with information that his parents are Hmong and came to this country at the end of the Vietnam war. You and I are faced with his educational salvation. You are a special educator or my consultant With your small group, respond to each of the following questions: 1. As special educator/consultants, what are some factors within the classroom that Joey’s teacher needs to consider other than his deficits? 2. In order to plan anything for Joey, what other questions would you ask? Wlurt other information would you need? Identify at least three questions. 3. In Joey’s community, we know that there is a) a community center which focuses on meeting the needs of Hmong and other immigrant families, b) several churches, synagogues, and mosques, c) social organizations (i.e., Lion’s Club, Boy Scouts), d) community recreation facilities, e) public library, 1) a zoo, g) several restaurants, h) light industry, and 1) several others. As teachers involved in developing an educational plan for Joey, how would you attempt to create linkages between at least four of these agencies and organizations that might result in improved educational outcomes for Joey? 4. Several of Joey’s peers call attention to his facial features (slanted eyes). List at least three things you would suggest to alter this behavior in the classroom. 5. There is a rumor that we may have mildly impaired classrooms next year. If you believe a referral for evaluation is need: a) Which category would you suggest Joey be assigned to? b) Why would you choose this category? If no choice is made by your group at this time, support your reason for not labeling him. APPENDIX B ABCD QUESTIONNAIRE 200 Name: School: (3mm vmammm 01min wmmumaummymumd mmlmmflflwe-r Emammmumd mum Will] dammmtnlflpmwlm filflkmmflfluflsmgfls Yumhlflsqfllauflrewm hmdylrytlcmdlfla m Yul Wuhan-yam rill-em Wewlllnrlshaethe MmythImywnflymafldlnMfldbymmym sin! \Veulll uhMmdhmflnmlfls Wm flpnmmbmflkmwdpaflflflem Ills-z 'Ihavereadadesaipuonofthethequesuonnaircandlunderstandwhatwillberequiredofme. I understand thatmy participation is strictly voltmtary arrdthatany informationl supply will notbe usedinways thatlcanbeidentified witlroutmy prior permission. " Signature Date I’mmfldmbMamdflcn-ibdflumymnmdh mhflsfladflnufldflem. 201 1. If you are a regular classroom teacher or planning to become one, please indicate the grade level(s) you currently teach or plan to teach. If you are a secondary teacher or planning to become one, please indicate the subject matter(s) and grade level(s) you currently teach or plan to teach. Elementary grade level: Secondary subject matter & grade level: If you are a special education teacher or planning to become one, please indicate the grade level(s) and areas of disability you currently teach or plan to teach. Grade level(s): Disability area(s): 2. Which of the following best describes the community in which the high school you attended is located? 1. Small town/Rural (pop. less than 25,000) 2. Town (pop. 25,000 to 100,000) 3. City (pop. 100,000 to 500,000) 4. Urban (pop. more than 500,000) 3. Which of the following best describes the size of the high school you attended? 1-250 students 251-500 students 501-750 students 751-1000 students 1001-1250 students 1251- 1500 students 1500-2000 students More than 2001 students @9991?pr 4. How would you describe your ethnic and/or racial heritage (for example, "African- American," ”Asian-American," "American Indian,” " Latino," ”Scotch-Irish," "Scandinavian,” "European-American," "African-American and Asian,” etc. ). We ask this because we are interested in the role that ethnic backgmmd plays in people's experiences and beliefs. 5. What is your gender? Male_ Female 6. What is your date of birth: mon. day year Views of Teaching and Learning For the statements below, indicate your agreement or disagreement by circling the number that best expresses what you think about the statement. Your replies to these statements can range from strongly agree (SA or 1) to strongly disagree (SD or 7). O o n o n o n Strongly Agree Not Sure Strongly Disagree 1 = strongly agree 5 = slightly disagree 2 = moderately agree 6 = moderately disagree 3 = slightly agree 7 = strongly disagree 4 = not sure SA SD 1. Teachers should avoid grouping students by ability or level of performance. 2. A lot of my ideas about teaching and learning come from my own experience as a student. 3. Teachers should use the same standards in evaluating the work of all students in the class. 4. It is impractical for teachers to tailor instruction to the unique interests and abilities of different students. 5. Students learn best if they have to figure things out for themselves instead of being told or shown. 6. When working with slow learners, teachers should focus nearly all their instruction on "minimum competency” objectives. 7. Required high school courses should have separate classes for low-achieving and hi gh-achieving students. 8. The main job of the teacher is to transmit the values of the mainstream American culture. 9. The main job of the teacher is to encourage students to think and question the world around them. 10. The main job of the teacher is to teach subject matter. 11. All students should be taught in English. 1 234567 234567 203 SA SD 12. There are some students who can simply never be good at writing. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 13. Some people are naturally able to organize their thoughts for writing. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 14. To be good at mathematics, you need to have a kind of "mathematical mind.” 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 15. When students are successful in achieving intended goals or objectives, that success is often attributed to one of the following sources. Which do you believe is the most frequent source of success? (Choose one.) l. Student's home background 2. Student's intellectual ability 3. Student's enthusiasm or perseverance 4. Teacher's attention to the unique interests and abilities of students 5. Teacher's use of effective methods of teaching 6. Teacher's enthusiasm or perseverance Briefly, can you tell us why you believe this? 204 16. When students fail to achieve intended goals or objectives, that failure is often attributed to one of the following sources. Which do you believe is the most frequent source of failure? (Choose mg.) 1. Student's home background 2. Students' lack of intellectual ability 3. Students' indifference or lack of perseverance 4. Teacher's failure to consider the unique interests and abilities of students 5. Teacher's failure to use effective methods of teaching 6. Teacher's indifference or lack of perseverance Briefly, can you tell us why you believe this? If you are neither an elementary teacher nor a secondary teacher of math or English, please SKIP questions 17-20 and proceed to question 21. If you are an elementary teacher, please answer questions 17-20. If you are a secondary teacher of mathematics, please answer questions 17-18. It you are a secondary teacher of English, please answer questions 19-20 205 17. If you were working with low achievers in mathematics which one of the following would you emphasize most? (Choose gn_e.) MPP’Nr‘ Basic computational skills Non-traditional topics, such as statistics and probability. Problem solving Helping students understand the theories behind the topics Making math class fun for students Other (specify): Briefly, can you tell us why you would do this? 18. If you are working with high achievers in mathematics which one of the following would you emphasize most? (Choose w.) l 2 3. 4. 5 6 Basic computational skills Non-traditional topics, such as statistics and probability Problem solving Helping students understand the theories behind the topics Making math class fun for students Other (specify): Briefly, can you tell us why you would do this? 206 19. If you were working with low achievers on learning to write which _on_e of the following would you emphasize most? (Choose org.) 1. 9MPP°E° Basic spelling and grammatical skills Non-traditional types of writing, like sonnets and editorials Developing and refining an argument in writing Helping students understand the role of audience and purpose in writing Having fun through composing things like haiku poems Other (specify): Briefly, can you tell us why you would do this? 20. If you were working with high achievers on learning to write which one of the following would you emphasize most? (Choose o_ne.) l. 2. 3. 4 S 6. Basic spelling and grammatical skills Non-traditional types of writing, like sonnets and editorials Developing and refining their ideas in writing Helping students understand the role of audience and purpose in writing Having fun through composing things like haiku poems Other (specify): Briefly, can you tell us why you would do this? 21. 207 Many teachers work in classrooms that include students from a variety of ethnic and social backgrounds. Below is a list of things that teachers might try to do in such classrooms. Circle the number beside the three things that you would be most likely to do if you were to teach in such a classroom. Then, rank these three things from 1 to 3 in order of their importance to you. Rank order things teachers might try to do (Circle the three you'd be most likely to do and then write 1, 2, or 3 next to each to indicate which you would be most likely [I], second most likely [2], and third [3] most likely to do.) 1. To make sure that all students have the opportunity to understand the subject matter in ways that increase their capacity to fr gure things out for themselves. 2. To honor and celebrate diversity by having students from different backgrounds share their foods, customs, language, and values with their classmates. 3. To teach the common core of values that all Americans, regardless of their background, share and on which our political and social institutions are built. 4. To teach students about the discrimination and injustice that various ethnic groups have encountered. 5. To make sure that, above all else, Q the students feel good about themselves even if they aren't learning what is in the curriculum. 6. To teach students that American society offers opportunities to everyone and that anyone who wants to improve his or her economic situation can do so if they work hard enough. Briefly, could you tell us why you chose the activity you ranked #1? 208 22. In recent years, school celebrations of Christmas have been banned by many school boards around the country--to the consternation of some parents. Which of the following is closest to you view of this situation? (Circle the number before the statement that is CLOSEST to what you believe.) 1. I think that in public schools there should not be any celebration of any religious holiday. 2. If we celebrate Christian holidays, we also need to observe and celebrate, in a similar fashion, Jewish, Moslem, Buddhist, etc., holidays. 3. I think too much emphasis is put on holidays in school; we need to put the time and energy that goes onto holiday celebration into school work. 4. Our culture is predominantly Christian and our children should learn these values. 5. I think that the argument that children who don't celebrate Christmas will feel excluded is unsound. 6. I think that the majority of parents support such Observances and their wishes should be respected. 7. Other (please explain): Briefly, could you tell us why you feel as you do? 209 23. As you know, there's a lot of discussion about students' use of various regional dialects in the classroom. Which of the views below is closest to your own? (Circle the number before the statement that is CLOSEST to what you believe.) 1. Because the purpose of schooling is to get all students to speak the same language, dialects such as black English have no place in the classroom. 2. While students may use various dialects among themselves and in classroom discussions, they need to use standard English whenever they write. 3. Dialects like black English are fully legitimate languages and appropriate for classroom discussion and for expressive writing like poetry, but students must use standard English in writing expository prose and formal speech. 4. Regional dialects (such as black English) are fully legitimate languages and should be accepted in classroom discussions, speeches and compositions on a par with standard English. Briefly, could you tell us why you chose the activity you ranked #1? 24. How would you rate the opportunities to learn that minority, special needs, and impoverished students experience in your school? (Circle one) 1. Excellent-minority, special nwds, and impoverished students consistently experience opportunities to learn equal to the opportunities other students experience. 2. Good-most of the time, minority, special needs, and impoverished students experience opportunities to learn equal to the opportunities other students experience. 3. Fair--sometimes, minority, special needs, and impoverished students experience opportunities to learn that are not equal to the opportunities other students experience. 4. Poor--minority, special needs, and impoverished students consistently experience opportunities to learn that are inferior to those other students experience. Comment: 210 25. A number of people feel that minority, special needs, and impoverished students--as well as girls in subjects like mathematics and science-continue to face unequal opportunities to learn. Do you think this is true? What makes you think that? If you believe that these students do face unequal opportunities to learn, what do you think ought to be done about it? Thank you for your assistance. APPENDIX C ATTITUDE SURVEYS 211 Social Attitude Survey Name For the statements below, indicate your agreement or disagreement by circling the number that M expresses what you think about the statement. Your replies to these statements can range from strongly agree (SA or 1) to strongly disagree (SD or 7). Your responses will be seen only by the principal researcher and will not be shared in any way that you could be identified. You may refuse to answer any or all of the questions; however, your honest responses would be greatly appreciated and most helpful to my research. Thank you. o n o n o A o Strongly A gre: Not Sure Strongly Disagree 1=stronglyagree 2=modcratclyagree 3=slightlyagree 4=notsure 5 = slightly disagree 6 = moderately disagree 7 = strongly disagree §A Q 1. My friendship network is very racially mixed. 7 6 5 4 3 2 7 6 2. Affirmative action programs in business and education constitute reverse discrimination. 3. I would feel O.K about my son or daughter 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 dating someone from a different race. 4. It bothers me that a racial minority person has 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 never been President of the United States. 5. Some racial minorities are more of a liability 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 than an asset to our country. 6. My closest friends are from my own racial 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 group. 7. Overall,1 think racial minorities complain too 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 much about racial discrimination. 8. It is better if people marry within their own 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 race. 9. Schools should promote traditional American 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 values as well as the values representative of the culturally diverse students in the class. 10. Schools should require all students to learn 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 about the contributions of various minority groups to our society. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 212 More minorities would be successful in this country if they were willing to work hard. White people's racism toward racial minority groups still constitutes a major problem in America. I am not prejudiced toward any minority groups. I would enjoy living in a neighborhood consisting of a racially diverse population (i.e., whites, Blacks, Hispanics, Native Americans, Asians). I would enjoy teaching in a school consisting of a racially diverse population. 213 Social Distance Scale 0 G G O O O O 7=Strong1yAgree 6=Mostlyagree 5=Slightly Agree 4=NotSme 3 = Slightly disagree 2 = Mostly Disagree 1 = Strongly Disagree I would have as regular friends Whites Blaclg Him Native Americans Asjgg 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 765432 I would have as close friends IM'M E! ! ll' .08 N .v E . 3' 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 765432 I would marry or maintain an intimate relationship with ELM M listings; W Ariana 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 765432 I would have several families live in my neighborhood 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 765432 I would teach in a school with a minority (10% or less) Efl' Ell 11' .m I! .v E . 5' 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 765432 Iwould teach inaschool with amajority (50% or more) 20.8 Ell If . ”.v! . 5' 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 7654321 1 APPENDIX D STRUCTURED INTERVIEWS 214 Structured Interview, Part I . What can you tell me about your personal background (family, hometown, personality, school experiences, other significant experiences, etc.)? «What is your father's occupation? mother's occupation? f ather's education? mother's education? --Where did you grow up? What was this community like? «What kinds of activities were you involved in during junior high and high school? «To what extent have you been involved in a church? Which church? Any community activities? . Why are you taking this course? (Why not TB 250?) What are you expecting to learn from this course? «Are these things that you need or want to learn? Why or why not? . Do you think that what you learn in this course will help you as a teacher? Why or why not? If yes, how? If you're not planning to be a teacher, how do you think this course might help you? . Please explain your answers to Question #21 on the questionnaire. Why is #-- your first choice? a) What makes you think this is a worthwhile and an attainable goal? b) Why would you want to honor and celebrate diversity? What makes you think that this would be a good idea? c) What are some of the ”common core of values” that should be taught? Why? What makes you think that this needs to be done? d) What is something you'd want to teach about discrimination and injustice? Why? What makes you think that this should be done? e) What makes you think that this is a worthwhile and attainable goal? 1) Why do you believe that anyone in American society can improve his or her economic situation if he or she works hard enough? What makes you think that this is something that should be taught? . For Question #22 on the questionnaire, please tell me more about why you feel as you do. What makes you believe this? (How do you justify this belief?) . For Question #23 on the questionnaire, please tell me more about why you believe as you do. How do you justify this belief? (What is the basis for this belief?) . For Question #25 on the questionnaire, please tell me more about why you believe this statement is or is not true. What evidence is there that this statement is either true or untrue? --Do you believe that students in other schools and other cities face unequal opportunities to learn? If so, what makes you think so? If not, why don't you think so . Why do most poor and minority students not perform as well in school as do most white students? What makes you think so? «How would you feel about teaching poor and minority students? Why? What if the school population was predominantly poor and minority students? Why would that matter? --In what important ways would teaching poor and minority students be different from teaching majority students? Why? 215 «Do you believe that the knowledge and skills necessary for effective teaching of poor and minority students are different from those necessary for teaching majority students? Why or why not? If so, how? 9. Vignette: Imagine that you have been hired midway through the school year to take over for a teacher who is going on maternity leave. During the first day, you notice a group of Native American students sitting together at the back of the class, while White and Asian-American students are sitting in front. The Native American students don't raise their hands to answer questions or to participate in discussions. Later, when you mention this to colleagues in the teachers' lounge, they tell you that the Native Americans are naturally shy and that asking them questions embarrasses them, so it's best not to call on them. What do you think of the teachers' explanation of the Native American students' behavior? How would you deal with the Native American students in this class? 10. What experiences have you had with people different than yourself? (in terms of ethnicity, disability, social class, religion, nationality, etc.) «What is the racial makeup of your hometown and your high school? «Do you have or had you had close friends from a different racial group? (Category 1, 2, & 3) «Have you had any minority teachers before this semester? «Since beginning college, what experiences have you had with minority students? «Do you feel (or have you ever felt) uncomfortable with members of a particular minority group? Why or why not? 1. 2. 3. 4. 9. 216 Structured Interview, Part II What were the most important things you learned in this course? Why do you think so? Do you feel that you have been changed in any important ways as a result of taking this course? If so, how? Why do you think so? What were the most valuable and least valuable lab/lecture sessions? Why do you think so? (Participants will be shown a list of topics from labs and lectures to jog their memories, if necessary) Have any of your ideas/beliefs about minorities changed as a result of taking this course? If so, what, how and why? Why do most poor and minority students not perform as well in school as most white students? What makes you think so? To be effective in teaching poor and minority students, will you have to teach any differently than you would with majority students? Why or why not? What have you Ieamed in this class about how policies and practices at the national, state, and district levels affect the education of diverse learners? Scenario: Imagine that you have been hired midway through the school year to take over for a teacher who is going on maternity leave. During the first day, you notice a group of Native American students sitting together at the back of the class, while White and Asian-American students are sitting in front. The Native American students don't raise their hands to answer questions or to participate in discussions. Later, when you mention this to colleagues in the teachers' lounge, they tell you that the Native Americans are naturally shy and that asking them questions embarrasses them, so it's best not to call on them. What do you think of the teachers' explanation of the Native American students' behavior? How would you deal with the Native American students in this class? Do you think that racism and discrimination are still major problems in our society? Why or why not? 10. Has talking with me this semester affected either your perceptions of the course or the way you think about any of the issues covered in the course? If so, how? Why do you think so? 11. Did it ever make any difference to you that you were being interviewed by a black person? If so, how and why? If not, why not? What about when racial issues were being discussed? REFERENCES REFERENCES Anderson, LM., & Holt-Reynolds, D. ( 1995). Prospective teachers' beliefs and teacher education pedagogy: Research based on a teacher educator's practical theory. (Research Report 95-6). East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University, National Center for Research on Teacher Learning. Au, K.H. (1980). Participation structures in a reading lesson with Hawaiian children: Analysis of a culturally appropriate instructional event. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 11(2), 91-115. Au, K.H., & Kawakami, A.J. (1994). Cultural congruence in instruction. In E.R. Hollins, J.E. King, & W.C. Hayman (Eds.), Teaching diverse populations: Formulating a knowledge base (pp. 5-23). Albany, NY: State University of New York. Ball, D.L (1989). Breaking with experience in teaming to teach mathematics: The role of a preservice methods course. (Issue Paper 89-10). East Lansing: Michigan State University, National Center for Teacher Education. Banks, J.A. (1994). Multiethnic education: Theory and practice. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Bar-Tal, D. (1990). Group beliefs. New York: Springer-Verlag. Bamdt, J. (1991). Dismantling racism: The continuing challenge to White America. Minneapolis: Augsburg Fortress. Barry, N.H., & Lechner, J.V. (1995). Preservice teachers' attitudes about and awareness of multicultural teaching and learning. Teaching & Teacher Education, 11, 149- 161. Bern, DJ. (1970). Beliefs, attitudes, and human afiairs. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. Bennett, C., Niggle, T., & Stage, F. (1990). Preservice multicultural teacher education: Predictors of student readiness. Teaching & Teacher Education, 6, 243-254. Bennett, L. (1987). Before the Mayflower: A history of Black America. Chicago: Johnson. Bethlehem, D.W. (1985). A social psychology of prejudice. New York: St. Martin's. Bogardus, E. (1925). Measuring social distance. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299- 308. 217 218 Bondy, E., Schmitz, S., & Johnson, M. (1993). The impact of coursework and fieldwork on student teachers' reported beliefs about teaching poor and minority students. Action in Teacher Education, 15(2), 55-62. Borko, H., & Putnam, R.T. (in press). Learning to teach. In R.C. Calfee & D.C. Berliner (Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology. Boyle-Baise, M., & Grant, C. A. (1991). Multicultural teacher education: A proposal for change. In H. C. Waxman, J. W. de Felix, J. B. Anderson, & H. P. Baptiste, Jr. (Eds.), Students at risk in at-risk schools: Improving environments for learning. Newbury Park, CA: Corwin. Bullough, R. V., Jr. (1991). Exploring personal teaching metaphors in preservice teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, 42, 43-51. Burstein, N. D., & Cabello, B. (1989). Preparing teachers to work with culturally diverse students: A teacher education model. Journal of Teacher Education, 40(5), 9-16. Bymes, D. A., & Kiger, G. (1988). Racial attitudes and discrimination: University teacher education students compared to the general student population. College Student Journal, 22, 176-184. Cazden, C. , & Mehan, H. (1990). Principles from sociology and anthropology. Context, code, classroom, and culture. In M. Reynolds (Ed.), Knowledge base for the beginning teacher (pp. 47—57). Washington, DC: American Association of Colleges for Teacher Education. Chinn, P. C., & Plata, M. (1987/88). Multicultural education: Beyond ethnic studies. Teacher Education and Practice, 4(2), 7-10. Cochran-Smith, M., & Lytle, S. L. (1992). Interrogating cultural diversity: Inquiry and action. Journal of Teacher Education, 43(2), 104-115. Cushner, K., McClelland, A., & Saf ford, P. (1992). Human diversity in education: An integrative approach. New York: McGraw-Hill. Davidman, RT. (1990). Multicultural teacher education and supervision: A new approach to professional development. Teacher Education Quarterly, 1 7(3), 37-52 Dilworth, M. (Ed). (1993). Diversity in teacher education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Dovido, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1991). Changes in the expression and assessment of racial prejudice. In H. J. Knopke, R. J. Norrell, & R. W. Rogers (Eds.), Opening doors: Perspectives on race relations in contemporary America (pp. 119-148). Tuscaloosa, AL: University of Alabama. Duckitt, J. (1992). The social psychology of prejudice. New York: Praeger. Feiman-Nemser, S., & Buchmann, M. (1985). The first year of teacher preparation: Transition to pedagogical thinking? (Research Series No. 156). East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University, Institute for Research on Teaching. 219 Feiman-Nemser, S., & Buchmann, M. (1989). Describing teacher education: A framework and illustrative findings from a longitudinal study of six students. Elementary School Journal, 89, 365-377. Florio-Ruane, S. (1994). The future teachers' autobiography club: Preparing educators to support literacy learning in culturally diverse classrooms. English Education, 26(1), 52-66. Garibaldi, A. M. (1993). Preparing teachers for culturally diverse classrooms. In M. Dilworth (Ed), Diversity in teacher education (pp. 23-39). San Francisco: Jossey- Bass. Gay, G. (1983). Multiethnic education: Historical developments and future prospects. Phi Delta Kappan, 64, 560-563. Gibson, M. A. (1984). Approaches to multicultural education in the United States: Some concepts and assumptions. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 15(1), 94-119. Gillette, M. D., & Grant, C. A. (1991). Discontent, multicultural education and some implications for the education of students. Teaching Education, 4(1), 79-88 Gollnick, D. M. (1992). Multicultural education: Policies and practices in teacher education. In C.A. Grant (Ed), Research and multicultural education: From the margins to the mainstream. (pp. 218-239). London: Falmer. Gomez, M.I... (1994). Teacher education reform and prospective teachers' perspectives on teaching "other people's” children. Teaching & Teacher Education, 10, 319-334. Goodman, J. (1988). Constructing a practical philosophy of teaching: A study of preservice teachers' professional perspectives. Teaching & Teacher Education, 4, 121-137. Grant, C. A. (Ed). (1992). Research and multicultural education: From the margins to the mainstream. London: Falmer. Grant, c. A., & Koskela, R. A. (1986). Education that is multicultural and the relationship between preservice campus learning and field experiences. Journal of Educational Research, 79, 197-204. Grant, C. A., & Secada, W.G. (1990). Preparing teachers for diversity. In W.R. Houston (Ed), Handbook of research on teacher education (pp. 403-422). New York: Macmillan. Grant, C. A., & Sleeter, C. E. (1986). Race, class, and gender in education research: An argument for integrative analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56, 195-211. Haberman, M. (1991). Can cultural awareness be taught in teacher education programs? Teaching Education, 4(1), 25-31. Hadaway, N. L., & Florez, V. (1987/88). Diversity in the classroom: Are our teachers prepared? Teacher Education and Practice, 4(2), 25-29. 220 Hale, J. E. (1982). Black children: Their roots, culture, and learning styles. Provo, UT: Brigham Young University. Heath, SB. (1982). Questioning at home and at school: A comparative study. In G. Spindler (Ed), Doing the ethnography of schooling (pp. 102-131). New York: Cambridge University. Hilliard, A. G. (1974). Restructuring teacher education for multicultural imperatives. In W. A. Hunter (Ed), Multicultural education through competency-based teacher education (pp. 40-55). Washington, DC: American Association of Colleges for Teacher Education. Holland, R. P. (1989). Learner characteristics and learner performance: Implications for instructional placement decisions. In B. J. Shade (Ed), Culture, style and the educative process (pp. 167-183). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Hollingsworth, S. (1989). Prior beliefs and cognitive change in learning to teach. American Educational Research Journal, 26, 160-189. Holmes Group. (1990). Tomorrow's schools. East Lansing: Author. Holt-Reynolds, D. (1991a). The dialogues of teacher education: Entering and influencing preservice teachers' internal conversations. (Research Report 91-4). East Lansing, MI: National Center for Research on Teacher Education, Michigan State University. Holt-Reynolds, D. (1991b). Practicing what we teach. (Research Report 91-5). East Lansing, MI: National Center for Research on Teacher Education, Michigan State University. Irvine, J. J. (1993). Making teacher education culturally responsive. In M. Dilworth (Ed), Diversity in teacher education (pp. 79-92. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Jaynes, G. D., & Williams, R. M., Jr. (Eds). (1989). A common destiny: Blacks and American society. Washington, DC: National Academy. Jones, J. M. (1972). Prejudice and racism. Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley. Kagan, D.M. (1992). Professional growth among preservice and beginning teachers. Review of Educational Research, 62, 129-169. Kleg, M. (1993). Hate, prejudice, and racism. Albany: State University of New York. Koehler, V. (1985). Research on preservice teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, 36(1), 23-30. Ladson—Billings, G. (1991). Beyond multicultural illiteracy. Journal of Negro Education, 60(2), 147-157. Ladson- Billings, G. (1993). Culturally relevant teaching: The key to making multicultural education work. In M. Dilworth (Ed), Diversity in teacher education (pp. 106-121). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 221 Ladson-Billings, G. (1994). Who will teach our children? Preparing teachers to successfully teach African American students. In E.R. Hollins, J.E. King, & W.C. Hayman (Eds.), Teaching diverse populations: Formulating a knowledge base (pp. 129-142). Albany, NY: State University of New York. Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). Toward a theory of culturally relevant pedagogy. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 465-491. Larke, P. J. (1990). Cultural diversity awareness inventory: Assessing the sensitivity of preservice teachers. Action in Teacher Education, 12(3), 23-29. Law, S. G., & Lane, D. 8., Jr. (1987). Multicultural acceptance by teacher education students: A survey of attitudes toward 32 ethnic and national groups and a comparison with 60 years of data. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 14(1), 3-9. Lortie, DC. (1975). Schoolteacher: A sociological study. Chicago: The University of Chicago. Lott, B. (1995). Distancing from women: Interpersonal sexist discrimination. In B. Lott & D. Maluso (Eds.), The social psychology of interpersonal discrimination (pp. 12-49). New York: Guilford. Lynch, J. (1987). Prejudice reduction and the schools. London: Cassell. Maluso, D. (1995). Shaking hands with a clenched fist: Interpersonal racism. In B. Lott & D. Maluso (Eds.), The social psychology of interpersonal discrimination (pp. 50-79). New York: Guilford. McDaniel, E. (1994). Understanding educational measurement. Madison, WI: Wm. C. Brown & Benchmark. McDiarmid, G. W. (1992). What to do about differences? A study of multicultural education for teacher trainees in the Los Angeles Unified School District. Journal of Teacher Education, 43(2), 83-93. McDiarmid, G. W., & Price, J. (1990). Prospective teachers' views of diverse learners: A study of the participants in the ABCD Project (Research Report 90-6). East Lansing: Michigan State University, National Center for Research on Teacher Learning. McGeehan, R. (1982). The relationship of selected antecedents to outcomes to training in multicultural education for preservice teachers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University. McGuire, W.J. (1986). Attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vol. II (pp. 233-336). New York: Random House. Moore, R. B. ( 1984). School systems perpetuate racism and don't know it--Can we deal with it? In Multiculturalism, racism, and the school system: Addresses given at a CEA seminar (pp. 35-49). Toronto: Canadian Education Association. 222 More, A. J. (1989). Native Indian students and their learning styles: Research results and classroom applications. In B. J. Shade (Ed), Culture, style and the educative process (pp. 150-166). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Myers, D. G. (1990). Social psychology (3rd ed). New York: McGraw-Hill. National Advisory Commission on Civil Disorders. (1968). Report of the National Advisory Commission on Civil Disorders. New York: New York Times. National Center for Research on Teacher Education. (1991). Final report. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University, National Center for Research on Teacher Education. Noordhoff, K., & Kleinfeld, J. (1993). Preparing teachers for multicultural classrooms. Teaching and Teacher Education, 9(1), 27-39. Oskamp, S. (1991). Attitudes and opinions (2nd ed). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Hall. Paine, L. (1990) Orientation towards diversity: What do prospective teachers bring? (Research Report 89-9). East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University, National Center for Research on Teacher Learning. Pajares, ME (1992). Teachers' beliefs and educational research: Cleaning up a messy construct. Review of Educational Research, 62, 307-332. Pallas, A. M., Natriello, G., & McDill, EL (1989). The changing nature of the disadvantaged population: Current dimensions and future trends. Educational Researcher, 18(5), 16-22 Pate, G. S. (1988). Research on reducing prejudice. Social Education, 52, 287-289. Pepper, F. C., & Henry, S. I. (1989). Social and cultural effects on Indian learning style: Classroom implications. In B. J. Shade (Ed), Culture, style and the educative process (pp. 3342). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Petty, R.E., & Cacioppo, J.T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown. Ponterotto, J. G., & Pedersen, P. B. (1993). Preventing prejudice: A guide for counselors and educators. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Reed, DE (1993). Multicultural education for preservice students. Action in Teacher Education, 15(3), 27-34. Richardson, V. (1996). The role of attitudes and beliefs in learning to teach. In J. Sikula (Ed), The handbook of research on teacher education (2nd ed). New York: Macmillan. Rokeach, M. (1976). Beliefs, attitudes, and values: A theory of organization and change. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Ross, D. D., & Smith, W. (1992). Understanding preservice teachers' perspectives on diversity. Journal of Teacher Education, 43(2), 94-1(B. 223 Sampson, E. E. (1991). Social worlds, personal lives: An introduction to social psychology. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Saracho, O. (1989). Cultural differences in the cognitive style of Mexican American students. In B. J. Shade (Ed. ), Culture, style and the educative process (pp. 129- 136). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Saracho, O. N., & Gerstl, C. K. (1991). Learning differences among at-risk minority students. In H.C. Waxman, J. W. de Felix, J. E. Anderson, & H. P. Baptiste, Jr. (Eds.), Students at risk in at-risk schools (pp. 105- 135). Newbury Park, CA: Corwin. Sarap, Madan. (1991). Education and the ideologies of racism. Stoke-on-Trent, England: Trentlram. Sarason, SB, Davidson, K.S., & Blatt, B. (1986). The preparation of teachers: An unstudied problem in education (Rev. ed). Cambridge, MA: Brookline. Scheibe, KE. (1970). Beliefs and values. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Schwartz, B. N., & Disch, R. (1970). White racism: Its history, pathology, and practice. New York: Dell. Shade, B. J. (1989a). Cognitive style: What is it? In B. J. Shade (Ed), Culture, style and the educative process (pp. 87-93). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Shade, B. J. (1989b). Culture and learning style within the Afro-American community. In B. J. Shade (Ed), Culture, style and the educative process (pp. 16-32). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Shade, B. J. (Ed). (1989c). Culture, style and the educative process. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Shuell, T. J. (1986). Cognitive conceptions of learning. Review of Educational Research, 56, 411-436. Smylie, MA. (1989). Teachers' views of the effectiveness of sources of learning to teach. Elementary School Journal, 89, 543-558. Sniderrnan, P.M., & Tetlock, PE. (1986). Symbolic racism: Problems of motive attribution in political analysis. Journal of Social Issues, 42(2), 129-150. Stephan, W.G. (1986). Integroup relations. In G. Lindzey & E Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vol. II (pp. 599-658). New York: Random House. , Stone, BA. (1987). Learning to teach: Improving teacher education. Childhood Education, 63, 370-376. Tran, M.T., Young, R.L., & Di Leila, JD. (1994). Multicultural education courses and the student teacher. Elinrinating stereotypical attitudes in our ethnically diverse classroom. Journal of Teacher Education, 45(2), 183-189. 224 Trent, W. (1990). Race and ethnicity in the teacher education curriculum. Teachers College Record, 91, 361-369. Triandis, H.C. (1971). Attitude and attitude change. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Villegas, A. M. (1988). School failure and cultural mismatch: Another view. Urban Review, 20, 253-265. Weinstein, C. (1990). Prospective elementary teachers' beliefs about teaching: Implications for teacher education. Teaching & Teacher Education, 6, 279-290. Zeichner, K. M. (1993). Educating teachers for cultural diversity. (NCRTL Special Report). East Lansing, MI: National Center for Research on Teacher Learning, Michigan State University. Zimbardo, P.G., & Leippe, MR. (1991). The psychology of attitude change and social influence. Philadelphia: Temple University. Zimpher, N. L., & Ashbum, E A. (1993). Countering parochialism in teacher candidates. In M. Dilworth (Ed), Diversity in teacher education (pp. 40-62). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.