I. 0.»: 1‘0 4 a... 1.2. . t. «1a. .n 1.9.2114. Emu: “K1533... n , .T e a? .. :. .5: i, , a a a . ‘4‘ 1 .nfi . .l ¢ln .- etyaa, : ...:m.. _ . ‘ . «infihm 0‘ . , , 5.1. Q‘s-c'uutl ¢ ell; {III .. agafirgai 2: $3 .. 5:51;; ‘ . #5:," . that? ll 'r I‘l I I III, .4 ' I D [r Date MICHIGAN STATE UN resumes l l mm mm: III I II I! I II ’I , I l 3 1293 01562 1182 ll IVEFISI l l LIBRARY Mlchlgan State Unlversity This is to certify that the thesis entitled PROCESSING PARALLEL STRUCTURE IN READING AND LISTENING presented by MICHAEL D. ANES has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for MA degree in Psychology KW [Ml/u Aer; Major professor Fernanda Ferreira 12/16/96 0—7 639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE II RETURN BOX to roman thh checkout mm your moord. TO AVOID FINES return on or baton date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE PROCESSING PARALLEL STRUCTURE IN READING AND LISTENING By Michael D. Anes A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1996 ABSTRACT PROCESSING PARALLEL STRUCTURE IN READING AND LISTENING By Michael D. Anes Following a sentence fragment such as Sherlock Holmes noticed, the phrase the man's shoes is syntactically ambiguous. The phrase could be a direct object ending the sentence or it could start an embedded Clause, the man's shoes were muddy (a sentence complement construction). Experiments were conducted to determine the beneficial effects of repeating the above temporarily ambiguous structures in reading and listening. Contrary to previous findings, repetition of the object construction resulted in greater processing benefit than repetition of the sentence complement contruction in both self-paced reading and self-paced listening tasks. In the listening task, repetition of the sound, or prosodic, structure had no influence. Sensibility ratings by another subject group showed that the object constructions were less sensible than sentence complement constructions, providing a possible explanation for the processing differences. The role of semantic, syntactic and prosodic variables in the processing of repeated structures is discussed. Dedicated to Louise J. Anes, whose life and work will always be a part of me. iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank the members of my committee for their help and constructive criticism; Fernanda Ferreira, John Henderson, Barbara Abbott, and particularly Rose Zacks for her expert guidance through the last stages of this project. The love and patient support of Ann Garrett-Anes has enabled me to complete this work, and I am very grateful. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES INTRODUCTION I. Is Prosody Used IH'TAQIQBIGQHAQ 'és'r'rigééiiér'I's'IBHJéfff. II. ls Prosody Used in Parsing Tasks?...... Ill. Processing Parallel Structure EXPERIMENT1 I. Method II. Results Ill. DISCUSSIon .. EXPERIMENT2 I. Method II. Results Ill. DISCUSSIon GENERAL DISCUSSION .... l. Structural Effects in the éiééiifiéaiéLlll2212'.IZ'.'.'.'.'.'.'.’.I'.'.'.'.'.2'. ll. Prosodic Effects: Where are They?... .. APPENDIX A Experimental Sentences.................. APPENDIX 8 Main Verb Sentence Complement l Noun Phrase Preference Normative Judgments ................... APPENDIX C Tables APPENDIX D Figures.................. vii viii .30)“; 62 65 71 ListofReferences......................... 76 LIST OF TABLES TABLE 1 Frazier et al. (1984) - Experimental Sentence Conditions....... 66 TABLEZ Experimental Sentence CondItIons 67 TABLES Experimental Conditions - Experiment 2... 68 TABLE 4 Correlations of Difference Time and Sensibility Judgments... 69 TABLE 5 Correlations of Difference Time and Sensibility Judgment Parallel Benefit..70 vii LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE 1 SGnSIbIIItyJUdgments 71 FIGURE 2 Experiment 1 Reading Time-Second Clause Disambiguating Region ........ 72 FIGURE 3 Experiment 1 Reading Time-Second Clause lnterchanging Region .......... 73 FIGURE 4 Stimulus Sentence Construction-Experiment 2... 74 FIGURE 5 Experiment 2 Structural Parallelism Benefit-Second Clause lnterchanging Region......... ..........75 viii Introduction Prosody is a set of suprasegmental phonological characteristics of speech, such as amplitude, duration, and pitch, that speakers produce in addition to segmental features making up the words of an utterance. These attributes are perceived by listeners as word and sentence stress, rhythm, and intonation. In the following sections, evidence that the prosodic characteristics of sentences are identified by listeners is reviewed, as are the results of research of the relationship between syntactic and prosodic structure in spoken language. In these studies, sentences with two Clauses were manipulated so that Clauses had the same or different syntactic and prosodic structures, and the processing benefit of parallel syntactic and prosodic structure was measured. The objective of the present experiments was to determine if prosody is immediately used by comprehenders to assign syntactic structure to what they hear. Since Lehiste's (1970) pioneering work, investigators have been concerned with the question of the reliabilty of prosodic information in spoken language and the potential benefit any regular cues might confer in comprehension. If regularities are found in the way in which speakers render a given syntactic structure prosodically, researchers can manipulate recordings of these structures and employ a comprehension task that, it is hoped, can discover aspects of performance contingent upon prosodic cues in the speech stream. In this way, we can begin to understand how and when comprehenders register such information. The first focus, then, has been to submit productions of speech to acoustic analysis. These productions are obtained in various contexts. Sometimes they are recordings of spontaneous speech, though they more often consist of readings of sets of sentences selected specifically to be of a certain phonological and syntactic structure. Cooper (1980), Lehiste (1970), and Pike (1945), have shown that the fundamental frequency (intonation) of a broad class of declarative sentences peaks early in phrases and falls at the ends of phrases. F erreira (1993) has provided an account of speakers' phrase- final segmental lengthening and pausing found in descriptive phrases such as THE GREEN TABLE. These pitch patterns may be a source of information listeners use to determine the syntactic structure of what they hear. Some researchers (Beach, 1988; Cooper and Sorensen, 1981) have found distinct pitch contours in different syntactic structures, including sentences in which a phrase is temporarily ambiguous in its syntactic constituency. In the temporarily ambiguous structure below, the bracketed portion of the sentence could be a direct object of the verb noticed, or could be the start of a new sentence complement. In 1b), the presence of the verb were disambiguates the preceding Noun Phrase (NP) as the start of a complement sentence. 1a) Sherlock Holmes noticed [the man's muddled brown shoes] NP Object 1b) Sheltock Holmes noticed [the man's shoes were muddy] Sentence Complement Pitch and pausing cues to syntactic structure in production are intriguing, yet precisely how comprehenders use prosodic information is still an open question. One major consideration in the present study was the task to be used. Methods which reveal processing implications of stimulus changes as they occur in real-time, with a minimum of intrusion, are considered "on-line". On- Iine methods should show, for example, effects of integrating disparate information types or of processing material of differing complexity. Effects should be seen soon after presentation of the relevant input. Off-line methods, however, may show effects of stimulus changes but do not give an accurate account of their time course. A new, on-Iine sentence processing task, the Auditory Moving Window (Ferreira, Henderson, Anes, Weeks, Jr., 8 McFarlane, 1996; Ferreira, Anes, & Horine,1996), is used in the present study to examine the possible influence of both syntactic and prosodic structure in sentence processing. I. Is Prosody Used in Tasks of General Comprehension? The presence of prosodic information has been shown to facilitate predicting sentence length in a sentence gating task (Grosjean, 1983), where a portion of a sentence was played to participants and they were asked to press a button when they thought the sentence would end. In addition, detecting word-initial phoneme targets is speeded as prosodic information increases with sentence length (Shields, MCHugh, and Martin, 1974), when targets are preceded by appropriate prosodic contours (Cutler, 1976) and as semantic focus (in the form of prosodic cues such as lexical stress) is given to a lexical item (Cutler and Fodor, 1979). All of these results have been taken as evidence that prosodic information is identified and used in some general aspects of sentence processing, such as preparing for a specified amount of upcoming information or to become aware more quickly of semantically salient portions of a sentence. Speer, Crowder, and Thomas (1993) investigated a claim, originally made by Slowiaczek (1981 ), that prosodic structure helps to package constituents in short-term memory, and that prosodic structures are stored in long-term memory. Speer et al. recorded two versions of structurally ambiguous sentences, called syntactic-Change sentences, each with-appropriate prosody. Contrastive stress was added to two different words in a new set of recordings to create semantic focus-change sentences, where syntactic structure does not change. A set of recognition sentences was created, holding either prosody or words constant. Subjects listened to sentences and judged each as new or old (they had/had not heard the words before), and gave a confidence rating for their judgment. Recognition accuracy in matching conditions (previously presented words and prosody) was significantly greater than in mismatching conditions (new words or prosody) for both syntactic— and focus-change sentences. The cost of a mismatch was the same for both sentence types, and similar results were found with nonsense syllable strings in presentation and recognition sets. Speer et al. claimed that the recognition process was not based on sentence content alone — prosody was maintained in memory, providing a distinct recognition advantage to same prosody recognition probes. Memory for prosody of focus-change stimuli, where syntactic structure does not change, was as great for syntactic-Change stimuli, supporting Speer's earlier assertion (Speer and Slowiaczek, 1990) that prosody is not a signal of syntactic structure in one-to-one correspondence. Speer et al.'s (1993) results are particularly important for researchers interested in prosody in parsing. A single prosodic production of a syntactic structure may or may not conform to a mean pattern if one exists, variability that may be moderated by semantic attributes of the utterance. More importantly, one prosodic structure may provide varying degrees of evidence for differing syntactic structures. One way to combat the inherent difficulties in investigating a processing system that is faced with so much uncertainty is to focus on specific situations such a system might face. In investigating prosodic effects in comprehension, researchers have attempted to reduce uncertainty by creating rigid syntactic environments conforming to theoretical notions of processing ease or difficulty, to eliminate lexical differences to the greatest extent possible, and to allow prosodic effects to have an influence. These were the goals of the research described below, where prosodic cues to syntactic structure were manipulated. ll. ls Prosody Used in Parsing Tasks? For years, researchers have examined the processing of structurally- ambiguous sentences in order to understand the mechanisms underlying syntactic structural assignment. Parsing is the process by which comprehenders identify constituent parts of a sentence and attach them into a structure compatible with the grammar of the language. By forcing comprehenders to encounter sentences with more than one syntactic stmctural alternative at a point in processing (a common natural occurrence, not limited to experimental situations), and observing comprehension effects, it is assumed that fundamental linguistic processes can be discovered. Because there are an arbitrary (and perhaps large) number of possible constituents intervening between ambiguity and disambiguation in sentences with temporary structural ambiguities, Frazier (1979) proposed two principles by which a single, simple syntactic analysis is pursued. If the parser attempts to incorporate a constituent and the resulting structure is ungrammatical, reanalysis will be necessary. Frazier‘s model is often called the ”garden-path" model, because of the possibility of initial, incorrect stmctural commitment. Frazier‘s first principle is Minimal Attachment, where the parser attaches syntactic constituents into a phrase structure tree using as few nodes as possible consistent with the grammar of the language. Under Minimal Attachment (MA), an NP that could be an object of a verb or a subject of an embedded clause should be incorporated as a direct object, because a phrase structure tree so constructed will have fewer nodes. If an NP is ultimately incorporated as part of a the start of a Sentence Complement (S-Comp), the sentence as a whole is termed Non—Minimal Attachment (NMA). Frazier‘s second principle is Late Closure, where the parser attempts to attach constituents to the currently open phrase, rather than create a new one or modify an old structure. Under Late Closure, a noun phrase that could be the direct object of a verb or the subject of an embedded clause should be assigned as an object, because the verb phrase can still accept incoming material. The result of applying these two principles is the same; the parser constructs the simplest possible analysis. Late Closure is potentially operable in sentences where there is a main clause, subordinate clause relationship between the ideas expressed. Early (sentence 2a below) or Late Closure (sentence 2b below) sentences often begin with a subordinating conjunction, and usually contain a comma to separate the clauses. ln Minimal Attachment, phrases do not exhibit semantic or syntactic dependence in the same way, and the ambiguity is not resolved with punctuation. 2a) Because Sam left, the party seemed dull. Early Closure 2b) Because Sam left the room, the party seemed dull. Late Closure Many researchers (Ferreira and Clifton, 1986; Ferreira and Henderson, 1990; Frazier and Rayner, 1982) have tested the garden-path model and the application of heuristics such as Minimal Attachment. Using eye-movement monitoring, long reading times on the disambiguating word of nonminimal attachment sentences have been found, as well as a pattern of regressions to early sentence regions. These authors suggest that long reading times at disambiguation are the result of misanalysis, and that regressions are the parser’s recovery process, indicative of a restructuring of phrases. Most of the research and theory-building done in the 1980's in parsing was based on evidence from reading, although the goals of this work were not to characterize modality-specific processing principles. Only recently have researchers begun to investigate the effects of prosody in parsing. Price et al. (1991) conducted match-to-context experiments where subjects listened to a member of a sentence pair and chose the context from which it came. Sentences with ambiguous prepositional phrase (PP) attachment sites were presented auditorily to participants, an ambiguity that is often subsumed under the principle of minimal attachment. Far attachment PPS (MA) were disambiguated slightly better than chance. For example, in sentence 3 below, either Laura or the man could be wearing the robe. If the man is wearing the robe, near PP attachment to the man as a modifier is a more complex structure than far attachment to the main verb, where Laura wears the robe. 3) Laura ran away with the man wearing the green robe. Beach (1991) investigated the NP object I Sentence Complement ambiguity by presenting sentence fragments in a matching task. Fragments were of two lengths: material up to an ambiguous NP, and including part of an ambiguous NP. An example sentence is presented below. 4a) The store manager knows/ peculiar II customers and their personal problems. 4b) The store manager knows! peculiar I/ customers can cause lots of work problems. The first slash in sentences 4a and 4b marks the position up to which sentences were presented in short versions, and the double slash marks the presentation including part of the ambiguous NP. Synthesized speech was used to control pitch rise and fall and segment duration precisely. Subjects matched fragments they heard (which had a prosodic pattern typical of the structure) to continuations of the sentences at a level better than chance. Fragment length did not affect matching performance. In another experiment duration and pitch values were independently varied (in the italicized syllables above) along a 5-point continuum so that segments were presented with constant pitch, or with varying degrees of pitch fall at the verb (knows) and rise in the next stressed syllable (in peculiar). Pitch and duration values in the middle of the continuum were presumed to provide ambiguous syntactic structural evidence, and those on the endpoints were thought to provide clear evidence for a single structure. Beach argued that duration and pitch effects indicated both information sources influence structural identification. Pitch and duration interacted such that an information source was especially effective when the other source was ambiguous (in the middle of the continuum). Beach concluded that "there is no 1:1 threshold mapping between one type of 10 prosodic pattern and identification" (p. 658) and that a probabilistic relationship exists between the registration of a (possible) prototypic prosodic pattern and matching the pattern to a syntactic representation. In summary, testing listeners' ability to use prosody to disambiguate structurally ambiguous material has been done largely with matching tasks (to paraphrase or to context), but these tasks cannot uncover the time course of registering prosodic information. Researchers see effects of duration and pitch changes upon structural disambiguation, and even that segment duration and pitch change prosodic components "trade-off" in the extent to which they provide evidence for a syntactic structure, but we are unable to determine how and when these variables are registered by the comprehension system. The same criticism applies to other tasks recently used to study prosodic effects in comprehension (with mixed results), such as end-of-sentence probing (Kielgaard and Speer, 1993) and mispronounciation monitoring tasks (Watt and Murray, 1993). Recently, more on-line methods have been used to study the effects of prosody in parsing. Marslen-Wilson, Tyler, Warren, Grenier, and Lee (1992) recently investigated prosodic effects in MA and NMA sentences using a cross- modal naming task. In this task, participants hear a portion of a sentence and name a visually-presented continuation word that appears at the offset of the auditory material. Fast naming times in comparison to a baseline are presumed to reflect an ease of incorporation of the continuation word. The authors 11 recorded auditory sentences in natural prosodic renderings, such as Sa-c below 58. The workers considered that the last offer from the management“ was a real insult. 5b. The workers considered the last offer from the management“ was a real insult. 50. The workers considered the last offer from the management“ of the factory. With the inclusion of the complementizer that, Sentence 5a is syntactically unambiguous. Sentences 5b and 5c are ambiguous up to the word after the caret shown in the sentences. Sentence 5b is NMA without a complementizer, and sentence 5c is MA. Marslen-Wilson et al. reviewed evidence of fast reading times on and after the post-verbal NP in NMA sentences with cornplementizers, indicating the effectiveness of overt lexical cues in reading. The authors suggested that if prosody functioned similarly to the lexical cues, there should be no naming time difference to an appropriate visual probe word continuation (WAS at the caret in the sentences above) in the NMA without Complementizer (NMA - Comp) and the NMA with Complementizer (NMA + Comp) conditions. Naming times to targets in neutral sentences (simple declarative sentences containing the word WAS) and to inappropriate continuations (violations of grammatical number, eg. WERE at the caret position) were baselines. Subjects also rated the appropriateness of the continuation word. Marslen-WIlson et al. (1992) found that naming times to continuation words were significantly faster in the NMA + Comp and NMA - Comp conditions 12 compared to the neutral carrier baseline condition (10 and 11 ms faster, respectively). In the MA condition, where the target is an inappropriate continuation, naming time was 14 ms above the neutral carrier baseline, similar to the grammatical number violation condition (24 ms over baseline). Attachment conditions did not differ statistically in rated appropriateness, although Marslen-Wilson et al. took numerically lower MA than NMA continuation ratings as a registration of some conflict between prosody and the continuation. The rating of grammatical number violation (inappropriate continuation) was near zero. The prosody of NMA sentences was as effective as a complementizer, and the authors suggested that prosodic representations are created quickly enough to be used on-line in processing. Another procedure that can be used to provide on-line information about the role of prosody in comprehension is the Auditory Moving Window task (AMW), developed by Ferreira et al. (1996).The AMW is an auditory analog of self-paced word-by-word reading tasks (Just and Carpenter, 1980). In one of Ferreira et al.'s experiments, high and low frequency words were embedded in identical sentence frames, and the AMW was sensitive to lexical frequency on the critical segment. in other sentences, immediate effects of a syntactic garden path were seen in reduced relative “sentences compared to active sentences that were identical until disambiguation. Mismatching prosody conditions were created by splicing, and post-disambiguation words were listened to longer if the preceding intonation contour was inappropriate for the 13 structure than if the contour was appropriate. All of these findings indicate the AMW might be a good task to use when manipulating prosodic and syntactic structure independently. Ill. Processing Parallel Syntactic Structure One way to study the parsing of temporarily ambiguous syntactic structures in the visual domain has been to conduct self-paced word-by-word and eye-movement monitoring investigations of the reading of these structures. in spoken language comprehension work, investigators have also presented sentences to subjects and monitored their performance on a variety of measures, which, as discussed, differ in the extent to which they can uncover the real-time nature of syntactic comprehension. Early on in the investigation of syntactic comprehension, researchers hit upon the simple idea that performance on a task may be enhanced if the syntactic structure under investigation is repeated. While relatively few studies have had as their aim the question of whether repeated occurrences of a syntactic structure result in facilitated comprehension, the notion that the processing of syntactic structures can be "primed" by prior presentations is enjoying a bit of a renaissance in the present literature. In the initial investigations of syntactic priming, Mehler and Carey (1967) repeatedly presented tape-recorded sentences of the same syntactic structure to subjects, whose task was then to perceive a target sentence in noise. 14 Perception was difficult for sentences with an unexpected structure, but not for sentences of the same structure as the preceding items. Frazier, Taft, Roeper, Clifton, and Ehrlich (1984) further investigated the facilitated processing of repetitive (or parallel) structures. These researchers used several types of conjoined constructions exhibiting two form variations, in a first clause/second clause self-paced reading task. Variations were syntactic (passive/active, NMA/MA, and shifted/nonshifted heavy NP) and semantic (agent/theme, animate agent/inanimate agent). Example sentences of the syntactic form conditions from Frazier et al. are shown in Table 1. Frazier et al. (1984) found facilitative effects of parallel structure for all the form variants tested. Facilitation was computed by subtracting the reading time per word of the second clause from the reading time per word of the first clause. In the syntactic manipulations, there were substantial parallelism effects for conjoined active sentences but not for conjoined passives, conjoined NMA sentences but not conjoined MA sentences, and for conjoined shifted NP sentences but not conjoined unshifted NP sentences. Semantic manipulations resulted in strong parallelism effects as well. More recently, parallelism effects in a variety of sentence structures have been examined. MacDonald and Schuster (1994) reported results from self-paced reading studies indicating that ambiguous reduced relative clauses are comprehended more easily following both morphologically unambiguous reduced relative clauses (using participle forms like shown) and unreduced 15 relative clauses. Branigan, Pickering and Stewart (1995) tested a host of syntactic structures in parallel and non-parallel environments, including reduced relative-main Clause ambiguities (with the same results as MacDonald and Schuster [1994]) and NP objectlS-complement ambiguities (MA/NMA). The NP object/S-complement ambiguity did not result in any significant parallel effects, despite the use of full-clause self-paced reading, as used by Frazier et al. (1984). Branigan et al. suggest that although NMA structures are more syntactically complex and thus are initially less preferred by the parser than MA structures, NMA structures may cause too weak of a garden path for the parser to benefit much from initially postulating an NMA analysis of a second clause. Alternatively, reanalysis may occur rather quickly in such structures and parallel structure benefit is observed only in more difficult cases. The following studies are intended to address issues of concern in previous syntactic priming research. One point of some importance is whether the MAINMA ambiguity is fruitful to investigate given the history of equivocal processing results. The present experiments are an attempt to extend the reach of prior investigation to include another type of structural repetition, namely of prosodic structure, and to fix methodological problems in Frazier et al. (1984). Among the methodological problems in the Frazier et al. work is the fact that full clause reading times do not provide information on how quickly mechanisms that result in facilitation are activated. Also, in Frazier et al., MA regions are much shorter than NMA regions, so analyses of the second Clauses 16 are conducted after comprehenders have received differing amounts of material, and not only material of differing syntactic complexity. Finally, in Frazier et al., lexical frequency of the constituent words in the critical regime is not controlled. With analysis regions controlled on these factors, full region reading times, as well as those for shorter segments, may be compared. The present study allowed comparisons of processing time on second clauses of two-clause sentences that were parallel or non-parallel in structure. The first experiment is intended to replicate Frazier et al. (1984) using self- paced segment-by-segment reading and better-controlled stimuli. The AMW was used in the second study, using as a dependent measure the amount of time subjects listened to a segment after its offset. Ferreira et al. (1996) suggested that as there is no necessarily linear relationship between the length of an auditory segment and the time after offset required to comprehend the segment, segment duration should be subtracted from segment listening time (the time between button presses to receive new segments) for an accurate representation of processing ease or difficulty. Subtracting the segment duration from the listening time results in a measure termed the Difference Time, which were computed for each second clause segment. In the second experiment, prosodic and syntactic Characteristics of the first clause were fully crossed. Both the structure and prosody of the first clause, and the structure of the second clause, were varied independently. This design allowed the determination of several effects concerning the way in 17 which prosody is registered and used in spoken language processing. The cost (or benefit) of a mismatch (or match) of first clause prosodic and syntactic structure can be computed by subtracting the processing time for the parallel cases from the corresponding non-parallel cases. Prosodic parallelism effects can also be determined, for example, if the prosody of the first Clause produces an advantage in same prosody (and structure) second clauses, even if the first clause has a conflicting syntactic structure. Parallelism effects may only become apparent if prosodic and syntactic structure match, e.g. only if all sources of information in the first clause "point to" a single analysis of the second Clause. An interaction of first clause prosody, first clause type, and second clause structure in the disambiguating region would constitute strong, on-line evidence of the immediate use of prosodic structure in parsing. Self-paced reading and listening times were compared to show similarities in syntactic and prosodic contributions to processing time. Because presentation segments were the same, and both tasks required identical motor movements and used the same comprehension task, visual and auditory comprehension were more directly compared than in other work, where modality comparisons are confounded with substantial differences in task demands. 18 Experiment 1 I. Method Subjects. Subjects were 48 Michigan State University students enrolled in undergraduate psychology courses, who were given course credit for their participation. All subjects were unaware of the specific purposes of the experiment, had normal or corrected eyesight and normal hearing, and were native speakers of American English. Stimuli. Twenty-four experimental sentences were constructed, each a two clause conjoined sentence (with the conjunction and). Twelve sentences were modifications of Frazier et al.'s (1984) sentences. Each clause was either minimally or nonminimally attached with respect to the attachment of a post- verbal noun phrase. Within each sentence, all portions of the sentence after the main verb (the interchanging region) of MA and NMA clauses were equated in the number of words and syllables by adding prenominal adjectives. Across all sentences, first and second clause interchanging regions were matched in character length and summed lexical frequency of component words. Perrnuting clause position and the attachment variable resulted in four experimental conditions: first clause MA/second clause MA, first clause MAIsecond Clause NMA, first clause NMA/second clause MA, and first clause NMA/second clause NMA. Table 2 shows an example sentence in each of the four conditions of the experiment, with interchanging regions delimited by carets (“). Experimental sentences are presented in Appendix A. 19 All of the main verbs for the experimental sentences were taken from the S I NP-complement norms obtained by Trueswell, Tanenhaus, and Kello (1993). Eleven were equally biased in their tendency to take an NP or sentence complement, that is, for each of them the ratio of one type of completion to the other was no more than 1.67 to 1. These equi-biased verbs had an average S- completion I NP-completion I other completion percentage distribution of 47/49/4. Nine other biased verbs were selected that had a completion (S I NP I other) average of 40/41/18. These twenty verbs were combined with repetitions of the four most equi-biased verbs so that the overall average completion preference ratings for the verbs was 45I46/9. Sentence Complement I Noun Phrase preference normative judgments are presented in Appendix 8. Because experimental sentences in this study were greater than 80 characters long, line breaks were necessary. Breaks were placed immediately before the conjunction. Thus the first clause was always the first line of the display and the second Clause was always the second line of the display, preceded by the conjunction. This display was chosen specifically so that when a reader's eyes swept to the new line, eye movement and button pressing disruptions would be confined as much as possible to the conjunction region, and would not affect processing the subject noun phrase of the second clause. The experimental sentences were segmented for presentation in self- paced reading. The first Clause was presented whole, then the conjunction on the next line of the display, and then segments which were either single content 20 words, or a determiner plus the following content word. Prepositions were presented singly, so that three-word segments (eg. with the umbrella) were not presented. Sentences ranged from seven to twelve segments in length. There were five sentence regions: the first clause, the conjunction, the second clause subject NP, the second clause main verb, and the second clause disambiguating word. In addition, reading time for segments was summed for two aggregated regions, the second clause interchanging region (which contained material after the main verb including the disambiguating word), and the full second clause. Analysis was conducted for all seven regions. Seventy-eight filler sentences were constructed. Twenty-six filler sentences were a single clause, constructed to be similar in length to the experimental sentences by adding prepositional and infinitival phrases. The remaining fillers were conjoined, two-clause sentences that did not exhibit the parallel structure of the experimental sentences. Comprehension questions for each of the experimental sentences and 16 of the filler sentences were made, to ensure that subjects carefully read the sentences in the study. Comprehension questions were questions about an action or participant in the sentence, or about some attribute of the environment described in the sentence. For example, the comprehension question for the sentence shown in Table 2 was 'Was the meeting place well hidden7". Half of the comprehension question were asked about some aspect of the first part (or first clause) of the 21 sentence and half were asked about some aspect of the second part (or second clause) of the sentence. Plausibility norms were collected for each clause of the two-Clause sentences to ensure that they would be understood by the undergraduate subject population, and that there were no semantic differences attributable to plausibility among the conditions. The experimental and filler sentences were rated by 72 undergraduate subjects on a five—point plausibility scale. Plausibility was defined as how likely or believable the events depicted in the sentences would be. A rating of 5 was used to indicate a highly implausible sentence, 3 a moderately plausible sentence, and 1 a highly plausible sentence. To eliminate possible effects of parallel structure on the normative judgment, each clause was presented separately. In some second clauses, the subject NP was elaborated because of a previously existing anaphoric relationship to the subject of the first clause. Clause position was a between- subjects variable, and clause type was within subjects. Thirty six subjects received the 24 first clause sentences for judgment (half of which were MA and half were NMA) and 36 subjects received the second clause sentences (half of which were MA and half were NMA). Twelve lists of sentences were prepared for the naming study, with sentences occurring in a different random order in each list. Two examples each of highly implausible and plausible sentences were shown to subjects in the instructions. 22 First clauses were judged marginally more plausible (mean=2.28) than second clauses (mean=2.46), but only by items, F2(1,47)=3.51, p<.10, MSe=7921‘. Second clause plausibility may have been adversely affected because of the increased length required in some second clause items to elaborate what had been an anaphoric reference to the first clause. Mean NMA clause plausibility (2.38) did not differ from mean MA clause plausibility (2.36), and did not interact with Clause position. Because registering the believability of an event may be secondary to constructing a coherent, sensible sentence representation, plausibility judgments may not reflect the kind of semantic processing required in normal comprehension; For this reason, the two clauses were conjoined to create syntactically parallel and nonparallel sentences and another norming study was done. Subjects were asked to rate "how easy it is to make sense of the sentences", using a five-point scale. A rating of 5 indicated a sentence was very difficult to make sense of, a rating of 3 indicated moderate sensibility, and 1 high sensibility. Four lists of the 24 experimental items were constructed and presented without fillers. Each list contained six items in each of four conditions created by crossing the first and second Clause type variable. individual sentences occurred in one condition per list. Thirteen participants rated each list. Sentences with NMA first clauses were judged more sensible than those with ' in all reported results, F1 refers to analyses treating subjects as random effects, and F2 refers to analyses treating items as random effects. 23 MA first clauses (2.28 vs. 2.55), F(1,12)=29.03, MSe=.032., p<.05 There was no effect of second clause type. For sentences with NMA second clauses, there was a significant sensibility increase (rating difference=0.69), t(23)=5.56, p<.05, when the first clause was NMA compared to MA first clauses, but no sensibility increase for MA parallel sentences, resulting in an interaction of first and second clause type, F(1,12)=65.50, MSe=.036. These results are shown in Figure 1, with each structural version shown with 95% confidence intervals. Eguigment. The experiment was run on an IBM-compatible 486-based microcomputer, with an SVGA color monitor. A button box was used to obtain self—paced presentation keypresses and question answers. The box contained a button labeled NEXT for proceeding to the next display segment, and keys labeled YES and NO to answer comprehension questions. We. Subjects were seated and the experimental procedure was explained to them. Subjects sat with their preferred hand resting on the pacing key of the button box. Subjects were instructed to read each segment normally, and that comprehension questions would appear after a random subset of the sentences. Subjects read twelve, two-line sentences for practice. Practice sentences were not parallel in structure, and were provided to give subjects practice pressing the button to display reading segments. After the practice session the group of 102 sentences was randomized for presentation for each subject by the experiment-running program. Each subject saw only one version 24 of each experimental sentence, but across items, received all experimental conditions. II. Results Results from reading time analyses on seven regions are reported: (1) the first clause, (2) the conjunction, and, in the second clause, (3) the subject NP, (4) main verb, (5) disambiguating word, (6) interchanging region (including all material after the main verb, including the disambiguating word). In addition, processing time for all segments of the second clause were summed and are also reported (7). Reading time for all experimental sentences is reported, including those in which there were comprehension question errors. Data including only sentences with correctly answered comprehension questions were analyzed and are reported separately. In all of the processing time analyses that follow, F values and significance levels are reported for the interaction terms of first and second clause structural factors. Where the interaction term is indicative of processing benefit resulting from parallel structure, contrasts on the simple effects were conducted to determine if a significant parallelism benefit for each structure (NMA and MA) was obtained. Unless noted, all p values for statistical comparisons are less than .05. Question Answering Accuracy. Analysis of comprehension question accuracy revealed a marginal effect of second clause type, such that questions about sentences with MA second clauses were easier to answer (89.1 percent 25 correct) than questions about sentences with NMA second clauses (85.4 percent correct), F1(1,47)=3.18, p<.10, MSe=.0201. No other effects approached significance. 1, First Clause. Reading times for the first clause as a function of the two experimental variables (first and second clause type), irrespective of question answering accuracy, were submitted to an ANOVA. Surprisingly, MA first clause reading times were 300 ms longer than NMA first clause reading time (4740 ms vs. 4440 ms), resulting in a main effect of first clause type, by subjects, F1(1,47)=5.41, MSe=798,284, and by items, F2(1,23)=5.18, MSe=481,419. This finding runs contrary to a body of results with NMA structures showing them to be more difficult to process than corresponding MA structures, and may have to do with the addition of words used to equate MA structures to their NMA counterparts in length and lexical frequency. There was no effect of second clause type and no interaction of the two variables. Reading time analyses for the first clause were also conducted after removing sentences associated with incorrect comprehension question responses. There was a marginal effect of first clause type, with an NMA advantage of 243 ms (4473 ms vs. 4716 ms), F1(1,47)=2.70, p=.10, MSe=1,051,278 and F2(1,23)=2.99, p<.10, MSe=534,917. Again, there was no effect of second clause type and no interaction of the two variables. 2, Conj'umion. The conjunction and constituted the initial region of the second clause for all experimental sentences. There was a marginal reading 26 time advantage if the first'clause was NMA of 13 ms (370 ms vs. 383 ms), F1 (1 ,47)=2.77, p<.10, MSe=3019, F2(1,23)=3.81, p<.10, MSe=1159. There was no effect of second clause type and no interaction of the variables. After removing errors, a marginal NMA first clause advantage of 12 ms was observed in the items analysis (371 ms vs. 383 ms), F2(1,23)=3.24, p<.10, MSe=1701. There was no effect of second clause type and no interaction of the variables. S. Subject NP. There were no effects of first or second clause type and the factors did not interact, either with comprehension question error items included or removed. 4. Main Verb. Analysis of all data revealed a marginal effect of first clause type. If the first clause was NMA, a marginal reading time advantage of 27 ms over MA first clauses (487 ms vs. 514 ms) was seen at the main verb of the second clause, F1(1,47)=2.71, p<.10, MSe=12,932, and F2(1,23)=3.06, p<.10, MSe=6177. There was no effect of second clause type and no interaction of the factors. In the analysis without errors, there were no significant effects. S. Disambiguating Word. A structural change is apparent at the disambiguating word, so this is the first place to expect effects of second clause structure. Second Clause structure may be consistent or inconsistent with first clause structure here; if either (or both) NMA and MA consistency (parallel structure) results in facilitated processing, there will be an interaction 27 of first and second clause type. Main effects of first and second Clause type are not interpretable, though, because the words to be compared are different. For example, in the example sentence in Table 2, reading time for wouldn’t in the first sentence and new in the second sentence are likely to differ. Thus, the interaction was examined to determine if benefit was significant for NMA and MA parallel sentences. in these comparisons reading times are compared for the same word as a function of the preceding clause type. In the disambiguating region, the interaction of first and second clause structure was significant, F1 (1 ,47)=4.54, MSe=15,157 and F2(1,23)=6.80, MSe=4646. At disambiguation in NMA second clauses, there was a nonsignificant 24 ms advantage if the first clause was parallel, while in MA second clauses there was a 52 ms advantage if the first clause was parallel, F1(1,47)=5.25, MSe=12,351, and F2(1,23)=6.59, MSe=5107, for the MA parallel case contrast. Figure 2 shows the pattern, with 95% confidence intervals for each condition. With question errors removed, the interaction of first and second clause structure was significant, F1(1,47)=5.61, MSe=20,225 and F2(1,23)=13.41, MSe=4615. At NMA second clause disambiguation there was no apparent NMA parallelism effect (9 ms). At MA second clause disambiguation, there was a 88 ms MA parallelism effect, F1 (1 ,47)=16.70, MSe=11,132, and F2(1,23)=5.24, MSe=4367. 28 S, lnterchanging Region. Controls over length and lexical frequency in this region allow main effects to be analyzed. Reading time after NMA first clauses was 87 ms slower than after MA first clauses (2304 ms vs. 2217 ms), F1(1,47)=3.25, p<.10, MSe=113,073, and F2(1,23)=3.39, p<.10, MSe=59,181. There was no effect of second clause type. The interaction of first and second clause structure was significant, F1 (1 ,47)=20.15, MSe=116,243 and F2(1,23)=18.00, MSe=65,066. In contrast tests, an NMA parallelism effect of 133 ms was marginal by subjects and items (2182 ms vs. 2315 ms), F1 (1 ,48)=3.20, p<.10, MSe=133,353, and F2(1,23)=3.07, p<.10, MSe=65,531, and an MA parallelism effect of 308 ms was significant (2426 ms vs. 2118 ms), F1(1,48)=23.79, MSe=95,957, and F2(1,23)=19.93, MSe=58,712. Figure 3 shows this pattern, with 95% confidence intervals for each condition. Analysis without question errors showed that reading time after NMA first clauses was 89 ms slower than after MA first clauses (2302 ms vs. 2213 ms), resulting in a first clause type effect, by items, F2(1,23)=4.38, MSe=56,960. There was no effect of second clause type. The interaction of first and second clause structure was significant, F1(1,47)=18.69, MSe=143,795 and F2(1,23)=12.57, MSe=112,948. An NMA parallelism effect of 148 ms was marginal in both subjects and items analyses (2171 ms vs. 2319 ms), F1(1,48)=2.82, p<.10, MSe=187,001, and F2(1,23)=3.16, p<.10, MSe=75,855. An MA parallelism effect of 325 ms was significant (2433 ms vs. 2108 ms), F1 (1 ,48)=23.83, MSe=106,506, and F2(1,23)=15.20, MSe=94,054. 29 Z, Second Slause. Analyses were conducted combining all segments of the second clause. No effects of first or second clause type were seen. The interaction of first and second clause structure was Significant, F1(1,47)=7.87, MSe=301,350 and F2(1,23)=7.97, MSe=162,477. A 159 ms advantage for NMA second clauses preceded by NMA first Clauses, as opposed to MA first clauses (4057 ms vs. 4216 ms) was marginal in the subjects analysis, F1(1,47)=2.98, p<.10, MSe=204,274. An effect of 286 ms for MA second clauses when preceded by MA first clauses (4013 ms vs. 4299 ms) was significant, F1 (1 ,47)=6.92, MSe=282,643, and F2(1,23)=8.40, MSe=124,255. No main effects were found in the analysis with errors removed. The interaction of first and second clause structure was again significant, F1(1,47)=6.89, MSe=394,028 and F2(1,23)=6.57, MSe=256,740. Comparisons showed that a 142 ms advantage for NMA second clauses preceded by NMA first clauses (as opposed to MA first clauses, 4053 ms vs. 4195 ms) was marginal by items, F2(1,23)=3.30, p<.10, MSe=124,280. An effect of 334 ms for MA second clauses when preceded by MA first clauses (as opposed to NMA first clauses, 4007 ms vs. 4341 ms) was significant, F1(1,47)=8.78, MSe=305,370, and F2(1,23)=6.29, MSe=227,570. Pogt-hgg Reading Time Analyses. After inspecting the experimental sentences, I determined that not all of the MNMA sentences were parallel in the structure of the interchanging region from the first clause to the second clause. For example, in sentence (6) below, the predicate NP of the first clause 30 is a simple noun followed by a prepositional phrase with a left-branching modified NP (that is, the adjectives modify the rightmost noun). The second clause also contains a simple noun followed by a prepositional phrase with a left-branching modified NP. The full sentence structure in sentence (6) is identical, node-for-node. In sentence (7) below, the beginning three words of the first clause modified NP are right-branching, while the final three words of the second clause modified NP are left-branching. Parallel NMA sentences, conversely, always had a consistent structure from first to second clause. Given the irregularity of the MA experimental sentences, I conducted analyses including a subset of nine experimental sentences in which the MA forms were parallel from the first to second clause, in other words, sentences like (6) below, but not like (7). (6) NP V Modified NP Friends in Washington mentioned the news about young man Clinton CONJ NP V Modified NP and other friends mentioned the rumors about old crook Nixon. (7) NP V Modified NP The CIA guessed the terrorists' very well hidden meeting place CONJ NP V Modified NP and the FBI guessed the location of the top secret new computer network. 1 First lause. The NMA advantage (121 ms), though present, was no longer statistically significant. There was no effect of second clause type and no interaction of the variables. 31 2, Sonjunction. The NMA advantage over MAS was robust (366 ms vs. 399 ms), F1(1,47)=4.48, MSe=11,412, and F2(1,8)=7.91, MSe=1066. No effect of second clause type and no interaction of the two variables was apparent. S. Subjeg NP. An effect ofisecond clause type reached significance in the items analysis, in that the subject NP was read faster (by 37 ms) if the second clause turned out to be MA, F2(1,8)=5.91, MSe=2010. No effect of the first clause type was found and there was no interaction of the variables. 4, Main Verb. A 42 ms NMA first clause advantage (473 ms vs. 515 ms) was reliable in the subjects analysis, F1 (1 ,47)=4.71, MSe=18,001. There was no effect of second clause type or interaction of first and second clause type. S, Qi§§mbiguating Word. First and second clause structure did not interact in this region, so contrasts were not analyzed. S. lnterchanging Rggion. There were no significant effects of first or second clause type. An interaction of first and second clause structure obtained in this region, F1(1,47)=15.30, MSe=144,353, and F2(1,8)=29.92, MSe=14,544. Contrast testing showed a nonsignificant NMA parallelism effect of 145 ms. An MA paralleliSm effect of 294 ms was reliable (2433 ms vs. 2139 ms), F1(1,47)=10.46, MSe=198,065, and F2(1,8)=8.68, MSe=55,536. Z. Smnd Slause. There was no effect of first or second clause type. An interaction of first and second clause structure was seen in this region, F1(1,47)=6.56, MSe=290,860, and F2(1,8)=7.57, MSe=48,280. Comparisons showed that a 216 ms advantage for NMA second Clauses when preceded by 32 NMA first clauses (as opposed to MA, 3850 ms vs. 4066 ms) was marginal in the subjects analysis , F1 (1 ,47)=2.96, p<.10, MSe=379,776. An effect of 183 ms for MA second clauses when preceded by MA first clauses (3939 ms vs. 4122 ms) was nonsignificant. Eost-Hoc Sorrelational Analyses. The direct objects in MA sentences in this experiment were quite complex. Because the internal object NP structure of the MAIMA sentences make them far less parallel than the NMA sentences (only 9 of 24 MNMA sentences had the same internal direct object structure from the first to the second clause), repetition of the exact structural tree cannot be the only level at which parallelism benefits are observed. Otherwise, strong syntactic parallelism benefit would be seen for the MAIMA parallel subset, and not necessarily for all of the MNMA sentences. in order to investigate the relationship between the processing difficulty of MAIMA structures and their inherent complexity, a series of correlational analyses were undertaken. First, reading time data for regions of the second clause (the main verb and the disambiguating and interchanging regions) and the raw sensibility norms were entered into analyses for each structural condition of Experiment 1. in this way, the relationship between the rated sensibility of a particular sentence and its reading time can be seen. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied based on the total of twelve computed correlations (four structures and three regions). 33 The amount of rated sensibility benefit for conjoining each sentence with its same structure second clause was also computed, and these data were entered into an analysis with the computed reading time benefit for each conjoined sentence in second Clause regions, starting at the main verb. This procedure allowed a determination of the relationship between ratings benefit from parallelism in individual tokens of each structural type and any resultant processing time benefit for those same sentences. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied based on the total of 6 computed correlations (two parallel structure conditions and three regions). The rated sensibility of a sentence did not significantly correlate with reading time for any of the four sentence types; In the sensibility norming experiment, only NMA parallel structures showed a mean sensibility benefit; that is, subjects rated sentence tokens as being more sensible in their NMAINMA versions than in their MNNMA versions. Overall, MAIMA sentences were not rated more sensible than their corresponding, non-parallel NMA/MA versions. Benefit in rated conjoined sensibility could conceivably be associated with processing time advantages, resulting in a relationship between the individual conjoined sentence sensibility benefit and the reading time parallelism benefit for each sentence. This relationship might be seen for MA parallel structures, even though as a set they did not show a mean parallel sensibility benefit in the norming study results. For each sentence, second clause reading times of the main verb, disambiguating region and 34 interchanging region of parallel sentences were subtracted from their non- parallel counterparts. Sensibility judgments of each parallel sentence were subtracted from that of the non-parallel version. There was no relationship between sensibility increases in NMA parallel sentences and any of the segment reading time benefits. For MA parallel sentences, however, sensibility judgment and reading time parallelism benefits mirrored each other, showing a strong positive correlation in the disambiguating region, Pearson r = 0.528, (x’=7.022, p<.01). ill. Discussion Two normative judgments were collected on the experimental sentences. In one of these judgments, single NMA and MA clauses were seen as equally plausible. in a second judgment, of the sensibility of the conjoined structures, sentences with NMA first clauses were rated more sensible than those with MA first clauses, and conjoined NMA clauses were rated more sensible than conjoined MA clauses. Reading times for the first clause showed unexpected difficulty for MA structures and relative ease for NMA structures. Early in the second clause, the conjunctions of second Clauses were read faster after NMA first clauses, a possible "spillover" effect of first clause ease. Benefit of MA structural parallelism was seen in reading time at disambiguation of the second clause, and for both structures in the aggregated reading times of second clause regime. Given the difficulty of MA first clauses in reading time and in 35 sensibility, it may be that if processing a clause is quite difficult, parsing another of the same type is easier. Parallel structure benefit is still observed in cases where structural assignment is not as difficult, as in NMA/NMA sentences, although effects are not necessarily immediate and not always robust. Interestingly, in analyses of full second clause processing for only those sentences with identical syntactic structure in the MA interchanging regions, the MA parallelism advantage disappears and the NMA parallel sentences show marginal benefit. These results are different than those reported in Frazier et al. (1984) and in Branigan et al. (1995). Frazier et al. do not report first clause reading time but we can presume, in line with F razier's parsing principles, the authors would have expected NMAs to be difficult. in Branigan et al. there were no differences in first clause reading time. Here, NMA first clauses were read for much less time than MA first clauses. Frazier et al. report significant second clause parallelism effects in NMA parallel sentences but not MA parallel sentences. The converse of Frazier et al.'s results is seen in Experiment 1, with parallelism effects for MAIMA sentences and trends for NMA/NMA sentences. Branigan et al., on the other hand, did not find significant parallelism benefit for either structure in NP objectIS-complement constructions. It is possible that the MA clauses were unusual in the present study as a result of equating sentence length in the object NPs with the addition .of prenominal adjectives. These additions led to variability in the internal object NP syntactic structure of 36 interchanging regions of MA parallel sentences, making them more difficult than their more consistent NMA parallel counterparts. in summary, the results from Experiment 1 indicate immediate facilitative effects of parallel structure in the reading time of second clause disambiguating regime. The effect is reliable only for the more difficult MAIMA structures. The processing time advantage of MA parallel sentences over NMA parallel sentences remains constant in larger analysis regions of the second clause. importantly, reading time benefit in the disambiguating region for MA parallel sentences was strongly correlated with the sensibility rating benefits for those sentences, but this relationship was not apparent in NMA parallel structures. Experiment 2 Experiment 2 was designed to determine whether parallel syntactic and prosodic structure affect listening, using the Auditory Moving Window task. Acoustically recorded versions of the stimuli used in Experiment 1 were used, with the prosody of each first clause manipulated to be consistent or inconsistent with the syntactic structure. As in Experiment 1, the full first Clause of each experimental sentence was presented as a single segment when the subject initiated atrial. By presenting the first clause in its entirety, l hoped to provide the best environment for subjects to use both the prosodic and syntactic structure available to them. Subjects continued to press the pacing button to hear the conjunction, and to hear each succeeding second clause segment. The conjunctim was a separate segment to allow greater within- 37 sentence practice so that subsequent button presses would be minimally dismpted. Presentation segments were a modified version of the prosodic word (Nespor and Vogel, 1987). Segments were content words unless preceded by a determiner, in which case the determiner was included with the following content word. Prepositions and the infinitival to were individual segments. Two tokens of each sentence were recorded, so that all first clauses of the experimental sentences consisted of two waveform portions combined into me clause. For mismatching conditions, an NMA syntax first clause was presented with MA prosody, or a MA syntax first Clause was presented with NMA prosody. By crossing first clause prosody and first clause syntax, I hoped to determine if and when prosodic and syntactic structure matching benefits are apparent, and if specific benefits are observed in second clauses with parallel syntactic and prosodic structure. The dependent measures were the listening time for a segment (the time between button presses) and the difference time, computed by subtracting the duration of each presentation segment from the listening time. Only difference time results are discussed here. I. Method Subjggs. Subjects were 48 Michigan State University students enrolled in undergraduate psychology courses. All subjects were given course credit for their participation, and were unaware of the specific purposes of the 38 experiment. Participants had normal hearing and were native speakers of American English. m. A male native speaker of American English with a neutral accent was recruited through an advertisement posted in the theater department. The speaker was unaware of the specific purposes of the experiment. The speaker was presented with the materials to be spoken, and was asked to familiarize himself with each sentence before reading it. Only one sentence was visible to the speaker at a time. The speaker produced each sentence and then listened to the recorded version. if there were no errors or hesitations, the speaker immediately recorded a second token of the same sentence. Comprehensim questims were also recorded for each experimental sentence. Two versions of each experimental sentence in each of the two parallel structure conditims (MAIMA and NMA/NMA) were recorded and digitized (at a 20 kHz sampling rate) using the Computerized Speech Laboratory software and hardware system (CSL, Kay Elemetrics, Pine Brook, NJ). Filler sentences and their cornprehensim questions were the same as those of Experiment 1. The speaker recorded all sentences in a random order. Waveforms for the eight experimental conditions were manipulated. Two splices were made into one token of each originally-recorded sentence, one at the beginning of the interchanging region of the first clause, and the second before the conjunction which initiated the second clause. Several steps were then taken to create the four conditions with NMA first clause prosody. A 39 pictorial representation of the experimental manipulations for NMA/NMA and MAIMA sentences is reproduced in Figure 4, and the experimental conditims are shown in Table 3. lnaudible presentation markers (tags) were placed in the waveform of each sentence at the end of the first clause, before the conjunctim, and at the end of each succeeding presentation segment until the end of the sentence. These tags demarcated sections of the waveform that the experimental program played to the subject, consecutively, with each button press. Questions were played out to the subjects as one segment. A 500 ms tone was appended to the end of each sentence waveform as part of the final segment, so that subjects were aware that the end of the sentence had occurred. Comprehension questions were played after‘the button press at the end of the last segment. Presentation tags were placed at low amplitude transitims between the last phmeme of a word and the first phoneme of the next word to create natural-sounding transitions. Cursor control of placement in CSL is very precise: each cursor movement can be as short as one-half of one millisecond. . The location of the first tag was manipulated in the filler sentences. By moving the location where segment-by-segment listening began, l hoped to reduce the predictability of segment lengths in the full set of sentences, and to maintain subject vigilance. if subjects could‘predict when self-paced listening would begin, they might not be as attentive to the content of the first segment. 40 Eggipment. Subjects' button presses were obtained with the computer and buttm box used in Experiment 1, and the experiment was conducted in the same location. The computer stored all of the digitized stimuli, and contained all of the digital-to—analog signal conversion hardware needed to present the stimuli auditorily. Subjects listened to the signal through headphones. Erocedure. Subjects were seated and the experimental procedure was explained to them. Subjects listened to fifteen sentences to gain practice in pacing through sentences with the pace key. Listening volume was adjusted for comfort during practice. The experimenter listened to the sentences through a second set of headphones. After practice, the 102 experimental sentences were randomized for presentation by the experiment-running program. Each subject heard only one version of each experimental sentence, but across items, received all conditions of the experiment. Subjects answered comprehension questions by pressing the appropriately marked buttm m the button box. M93. As can be seen in Table 3, the experiment was a 2 (Second Clause Structure) X 2 (First Clause Structure) X 2 (First Clause ProSody) factorial design. Given this design, several interactions may be obtained. First, a first clause prosodic matching benefit may be seen. This effect is the interactim of first clause structure and first clause prosody, collapsed over second clause type. if first clause structure and prosody are both NMA or both 41 MA, one might expect processing benefits (compared to mismatching cases) for these items in the first clause results, and perhaps in succeeding segments. Second, there is the possible beneficial effect of structural parallelism, compared to non-parallel cases, irrespective of first clause prosody. These effects (for NMA and/or MA parallel structures) are reflected in the interactim of first and second clause structure, collapsed over first clause prosody. Third, in this design I can determine whether the prosodic structure of the first clause exerts an independent influence on the processing of similarly- structured second clause material. If the first Clause prosody of an item is NMA, second clause structures that exhibit NMA prosodic characteristics in the second Clause may show a benefit over non-parallel (MA first clause prosody) structures. The same pattern might hold for second clause MA structures with MA first clause prosody. If either (or both) of the structural types exhibits the effect, an interaction of first clause prosody and second clause type, collapsed over first clause structure, will result. Finally, both syntactic and prosodic structure may influence processing of segments of parallel second clauses in comparison to non-parallel cases. Reliable effects in the three-way interaction of the experimental factors would indicate that processing of a parallel second clause (whether after NMA or MA first clauses) benefits from the joint influence of an introductory prosodic structure and a continuing syntactic structure. 42 II. Results Question flswering Accuracy. Comprehension question accuracy for the experimental sentences was computed and analyzed in an ANOVA Grand mean accuracy for the experimental sentence comprehension questions was 86.6%, and ranged from 81.3% for MA syntax/MA prosody/NMA second clause sentences to 91.0% for NMA syntax/NMA prosody/MA second clause sentences. There were no first or second cause main effects, no interactions of first clause and second clause type, of first clause type and first clause prosody, or of first clause prosody and second clause type. The interactim of the three experimental variables was reliable, F(1,23)=5.39, MSe=.0068. This interactim was the result of marginal prosodic matching benefits when second clauses were MA (F[1,23]=3.16, p<.10, MSe=.0058) and not when second clauses were NMA For MA second clauses, matching MA first clause prosody and structure resulted in a 1.4% question accuracy increase, matching NMA first clause prosody and structure resulted in a 4.2% accuracy increase. For NMA second clauses, however, matching NMA first clause prosody and structure resulted in a 0.7% accuracy increase, and a match of MA first clause prosody and structure resulted in a 6.2% accuracy decrease. This interactim is a reversal of a parallel structure effect, and is likely spurious. Evidence was not found for specific benefits in the case of all three variables in their parallel instances. 43 nggim Time Results: Difference Times. Results are reported below for the difference between the duration of the segment and the listening time. First clause duration did not differwith the structural or prosodic manipulations (all ts<1, grand mean=3343 me), so first clause and subsequent region analyses should be unaffected by the amount of time required to listen to the first clause. Acoustic analyses of the stimuli were undertaken to determine the prosodic characteristics of each clause type. Measurements were taken of both the subject NP and main verb of first clauses, which were presented whole and had a consistent prosody for a structure through about half of the first clause duratim (until the end of the main verb). This manner of first clause presentation was chosen to set up a prosodic representation without interruption from button-pressing, so that subjects would have the best chance to use any available prosodic cues in second clause processing. However, the comparisons of NMA and MA clauses at (presumably) the two most prosodically salient sentence regions showed no differences in duratim, mean fundamental frequency (F0), F0 range, or F0 standard deviation, all t statistics<1. The seven analysis regions and the types of analyses conducted (with queetim errors, errors excluded, and parallel item subset) are the same as those reported for Experiment 1. Analyses of the parallel subset are presented following the data with and without errors, in a separate section. Subject analyses are not reported for the post-hoe analyses of the fully parallel item 44 subset, because only nine items could be analyzed, and as a result each subject did not receive the full set of conditions. 1. First Slagse. Difference times of all data were analyzed. No effects of first type, second clause type, or of first clause prosody were seen, and no interactims approached significance. Likewise, there were no significant effects of any variable or interactions in the difference time analyses without errors. . z. Sonjunctim. The conjunction and was the initial region of the second clause for all experimental sentences. An analysis of difference times showed no main effects of the variables. A first clause type by second clause type interaction was observed, so that if the second clause (well after this region) turned out to be NMA, an NMA parallel structure effect of 49 ms was observed, and if the second clause turned out to be MA, an MA parallel structure effect of 68 ms was observed, F1(1,47)=4.41, MSe=74,813, and F2(1,23)=4.01, p<.10, MSe=41,206, for the interaction. Conjunctions at the beginning of NMA second clauses did not differ in duration, fundamental frequency mean or fundamental frequency variability from those in MA second clauses, all ts<1. Second clause structure is not yet known to the listener, and in the absence of any physical differences in the signal the apparent parallel structure effect seems to be spurious. Contrast testing was not done. First clause prosodic matching benefit was not reliable, and no other interactions reached significance. 45 Difference time analyses without errors showed no main effects of the variables. The first clause by second clause type interaction reached significance, F1 (1 ,47)=3.76, p=.05, MSe=91,667, and F2(1,23)=5.01, p<.05, MSe=34,543, reflecting an NMA parallel structure effect of 30 ms and an MA parallel structure effect of 90 ms. Contrast testing was not done. The interactim of first clause structure and prosody was marginal by subjects, F1(1,47)=2.77, p<.10, MSe=92,629, indicating some facilitative effects of matching first clause NMA prosody and syntax of 33 ms (621 me vs. 654 ms) and of matching first clause MA prosody and. syntax of 76 ms (572 me vs. 643 me), though contrasts showed both NMA and MA prosodic matching to be of no statistically significant benefit. No prosodic parallelism was seen, and the three-way interaction of the variables was unreliable. S, Subjggt NP. Effects of second clause structure, collapsed over the other variables, began to emerge in this region although the structure of the second clause was not yet apparent. Difference time data for all sentences showed that if the second clause was going to be MA, a marginal difference time advantage of 29 ms was seen over NMA second clauses (594 me vs. 623 ms), F1(1,47)=3.35, p<.10, MSe=23,814, and F2(1,23)=3.58, p<.10, MSe=11,131. No other main effects were apparent. Processing was facilitated by 32 ms for matching MA prosody and structure first clauses, and by 31 ms for matching NMA prosody and structure first clauses, resulting in an interactim of first clause type and first clause prosody by subjects, F1 (1 ,47)=7.95, 46 MSe=12,015. Contrasts indicated that the NMA prosody and structure match benefit was not significant. The MA prosody and structure match contrast was marginal in the subjects analysis, F1(1,47)=3.17, p<.10, MSe=15,148, and reached significance in the items analysis, F2(1,23)=4.47, MSe=5753. No other interactims approached significance. NMA second clause subject NPe did not differ in duration from those in MA clauses (431 me vs. 441 ms), t(23)<1. Difference time analyses without errors showed no main effects or interactions of the variables. 4. Main Verb. Difference time analyses revealed no main effects, and a trend toward a first claUSe prosody and structure matching effect, with an 8 ms advantage at the verb if the first clause prosody and structure were both NMA (423 me vs. 431 ms) and a 20 ms advantage (416 ms vs. 436 ms) if the first clause prosody and structure were'both MA, by subjects, F1(1,47)=2.86, p<.10, MSe=7078, for the interaction. Contrasts showed no effect for matching NMA prosody and structure, but a marginal effect for MA prosody and structure matches in the subjects analysis, F1 (1 ,47)=2.97, p<.10, MSe=6883. No other interactims approached significance. Main verbs in NMA second clauses did not differ in duration from those in MA clauses (370 me vs. 367 ms), t(23)<1. Analyses without errors showed no significant main effects of the variables. Processing benefits for matching first clause NMA prosody and syntax of 14 ms (422 me vs. 436 ms), and for matching first clause MA prosody and syntax of 30 ms (405 me vs. 435 ms) were seen, resulting in a marginal 47 interaction of first clause type and first clause prosody in the subjects analysis, F1 (1 ,47)=3.80, p<.10, Mse=11,997, though neither the NMA or MA contrasts were significant. There were no interactions of first and second clause type, of first clause prosody and second clause type, or of all three variables. S. Disgmbiguating Word. Second clause structure is determined at this point, but main effects of first clause type, first clause prosody, and second clause type are uninterpretable because the main effect collapses over different words. Facilitated processing of parallel structure is indicated by any interactions of first clause type, first clause prosody and second clause type. If significant interactions were obtained in the overall analyses, simple effects contrasts were done on the three analyses (all data, errors out, and parallel item subset) to see whether parallelism effects were significant within each structural type. The difference time analysis of all data showed no reliable interactims among the three experimental variables. In the analysis without comprehensim questim errors, all interactions were also unreliable. S. lnterchanging Region. Because character length, number of syllables and lexical frequency were controlled over the interchanging regim of the second clause, all main effects and interactions in the processing time data can be analyzed. Difference time results with all data included revealed a main effect of first clause type. Subjects listened to interchanging regions after NMA first clauses 98 ms longer than after MA first clauses (2274 me vs: 2176 ms), 48 F1(1,47)'=4.87, MSe=189,032, and F2(1,23)=5.79, MSe=79,457. No effects of first clause prosody were found, or of second clause type. A first clause by second clause type interaction was significant in the subjects analysis, reflecting a 16 ms processing time increase for NMA/NMA sentences and 180 ms facilitation for MAIMA sentences, F1 (1 ,47)=5.1 1, MSe=125,309. Cmtrasts showed no significant effect of NMA parallel structure and a significant MA parallelism effect, F1 (1 ,47)=1 1.70, MSe=132,230, and F2(1,23)=5.63, MSe=137,294. No other interactions approached significance. These results are shown in Figure 5, with 95% confidence intervals for each condition. inspection of difference times with error data excluded revealed a main effect of first clause type, with processing time of the interchanging region of second clauses after NMA first clauses 114 ms slower than the times after MA first clauses (2267 me vs. 2153 ms), F1 (1 ,47)=6.60, MSe=191,009, and F2(1,23)=8.23, MSe=99,078. No effects of second clause type were found. The interactim of first and second clause structure was not significant, yet contrast tests showed structural parallelism benefit for MAIMA sentences, F1 (1 ,47)=9.63, MSe=176,971, and F2(1 ,23)=6.76, MSe=159,300. No other interactims approached significance. 7, Sgcong Slguse. Analyses were also conducted for the summed difference times for all second clause segments. Difference times showed an effect of first clause structure in the subjects analysis: second clauses after NMA first clauses were listened to for 140 ms longer than after MA first clauses 49 (4326 me vs. 4186 ms), resulting in a main effect of first clause type, by subjects, F1 (1 ,47)=5.02, MSe=373,161. There were no effects of first clause prosody or of second clause type. No other interactions approached significance. Although the structural parallelism interaction did not reach significance, contrast testing was done for each structure to investigate possible parallelism benefit. Difference times for NMA second clauses after NMA first clauses were 10 ms slower than after MA first clauses: Clearly, there was no NMA structural parallelism benefit across the full second clause for NMA conjoined structures. Difference times of MA second clauses after MA first clauses were 269 ms faster than after NMA first clauses, resulting in significant parallel structure benefit for MA second clauses by subjects, F1 (1 ,47)=9.46, MSe=367,888, and F2(1,23)=3.17, p<.10, MSe=578,849. In the data without errors, there was an effect of first clause type, such that second clauses after NMA first clauses were listened to for 181 ms longer than after MA first clauses (4297 me vs. 4116 ms), F1 (1 ,47)=7.00, MSe=448,022, and F2(1,23)=5.51, MSe=479,154. There were no effects of first clause prosody or of second Clause type. Structural parallelism effects were marginal in the subjects analysis, F1 (1 ,47)=3.05, p<.10, MSe=882,753. Cmtrasts showed a nonsignificant 13 ms processing time increase for NMA conjoined structures, but 395 ms of benefit for MA second Clauses preceded by MA first clauses, F1 (1 ,47)=6.09, MSe=612,413, and F2(1,23)=12.51, MSe=465,147. First clause prosody and second clause structure did not 50 interact. First clause prosodic matching benefit was observed in the subjects analysis, F1 (1 ,47)=4.61, MSe=521,848. with a 163 ms benefit for matching NMA first clauses. and a 149 ms benefit for matching MA first clauses. Neither structure yielded significant levels of prosodic matching benefit in contrast tests. Beactim Time Results: Parallel Subset Difference Times. A subset of sentences in this experiment (nine of 24) were identical in the structure of the first and second Clauses. Data from this subset are reported below. 1. First Slause. Difference time analyses showed no effect of first or second clause type, or of first clause prosody. No two-way interactions were significant. The interaction of all three variables was reliable, F(1,8)=8.89, MSe=13,390, though likely spurious because none of the second clause had yet been heard. 2. Smjunctim. In difference time. no advantage was found at the conjunctim following either structure. If the second clause turned out to be NMA rather than MA. an advantage of 67 mswas seen in difference time for the conjunction (596 me vs. 663 ms), F(1.8)=3.49, p<.10, MSe=22.881. As in the full item set. there was a first clause structure by second clause structure interaction, F(1,8)=4.88. p<.10, MSe=10.413, with an NMA structural parallelism effect of 24 ms (584 me vs. .608 ms) and an MA structural parallelism effect of 82 ms (622 ms vs. 704 ms). Contrasts were not tested. 51 S, 4, S, S, Z. Subject NP. Main Verb, Disambiguating and lntermanging Rggiggg, ang Sflnd Slause. There were no main effects or interactions of the three experimental variables in difference time in these regions. Pgst-Hoc Sorrelatimal Analyses. Post-hoc correlational analyses of the relatimships between sensibility and processing measures were conducted on data from Experiment 2. In contrast to Experiment 1, there were statistically significant relationships between difference time and raw sensibility judgments in sentences with MA second clauses, whether parallel or not. These cases showed increased difference times for those sentences judged less sensible, across the main verb, disambiguating region, and interchanging region of second clauses. Table 4 shows these correlations. Correlational analyses were also conducted for the data transformed to show parallel sensibility judgment and difference time benefits. The amount of rated sensibility benefit for conjoining each sentence with its same structure second clause was computed, and these data were entered into an analysis with the computed difference benefit for each conjoined sentence in three second clause regions (the main verb, disambiguating region and interchanging region). This procedure allowed a determination of the relatimship between ratings benefit from parallelism in individual tokens of each structural type and any resultant processing time benefit for those same sentences. Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons were applied based on the total of 6 computed correlations (two parallel structure conditims and 52 three regions). in these analyses, the relationship between an individual item's savings in sensibility and savings in difference time for conjoined structures can be seen. There was no relationship between the two types of parallel benefit (sensibility and processing) for NMA parallel structures, but a robust relatimship for conjoined MA structures. This strong relationship between MA parallel sentences that benefit in the sensibility judgments and their difference time is apparent at the main verb and disambiguating region, and is shown in Table 5. III. Discussion In Experiment 2, subjects answered comprehension questions with about the same accuracy as in Experiment 1. In none of the region analyses did removing data from inaccurately answered questions qualitatively affect processing time results. in contrast to Experiment 1, subjects did not show differential difficulty with the clause types presented in Experiment 2. Difference times for NMA and MA first clauses were statistically equivalent. Not surprisingly, then, there were no spillover effects as a result of processing ease or difficulty of first clause structure seen in the initial regions of the second clause. Accounting for the discrepancy between Experiment 1 and 2 in first clause processing is taken up in the General Discussion. There were no significant effects of prosody in this experiment. Over the first clause, and all subsequent regions. there were no reliable effects of first 53 clause prosody that would indicate an intrinsic advantage for one prosodic structure over another. While there were trends toward prosodic and syntactic structure matching effects early in the second clause, they were never reliable. The present prosodic null results may be explained in part by the results of the acoustic analyses. There were no differences in the overall prosodic characteristics of initial regions of the first clause. One possible reason for this lack of difference may be that adding prenominal adjectives to increase MA clause length may have made MA Clauses less prosodically distinct than they might be naturally. Also, the speaker who recorded the sentences used in the study may not have realized that the sentences he was recording were of two structures and may have felt no reason to accentuate or prosodically disambiguate the structures; after all, he was not instructed to do so. in fact, sentences were randomized specifically to ensure that the speaker did not read different structural tokens of the same sentence close together in time, and prosodic cueing may have been de-emphasized as a result. Other factors may have contributed to finding no prosodic influence. Vifrth the level of experimental control over item characteristics in the study, it was very difficult to create the 24 items. in an eight condition design, each subject heard a given condition on three trials. Condition means for subjects, then, are likely to be unstable, reducing the statistical power necessary to detect higher-order interactions. Also. prosodic parallelism is defined by the matching of first and second clause prosodic structure, and in the present experiment, there were two inherent obstacles to finding prosodic effects. F irst, a first clause prosodic structure that is consistent with one syntactic structure is mly partially presented: at the beginning of the interchanging region, the splicing manipulation ensured that the next segments (and their prosodic characteristics) were from a sentence token of the other syntactic etmcture, in four of the eight conditions. In this way, only about the first half of the duration of the first clause contained the appropriate prosody that could "set up" prosodic effects in the second clause. Second, the AMW, by its nature, disrupts the flow of prosodic information to subjects. If more of the second clause had been presented with the initial second clause keypress, determination of second Clause prosodic structure might have been more successful and prosodic parallel structure benefits may have resulted. Structural and prosodic parallelism are each defined by interactions of two design factors (both interaction terms are collapsed over one other variable, doubling the number of observatims to six). yet consistent parallel effects are found for syntactic and not prosodic structure. In this study, it is Clear that syntactic structural factors were more influential than prosodic structural factors. MA parallelism effects in the larger analysis regions show a strong similarity to those seen in Experiment 1. In contrast to Experiment 1, however, NMA structural parallelism effects were not reliable in the disambiguating or interchanging regions. or over the entire second clause, but MA structural parallelism effects were strong in the two larger analysis regions. 55 General Discussion . The major findings in these experiments were that MA structures were more difficult to process than NMA structures, that there was greater parallelism benefit for theusually simpler version of a temporarily ambiguous structure, rather than for the more complex structure, and a lack of significant prosodic effects, either as a cost of mismatching or as an indication of prosody as a guide to parsing decisions. These major findings are all in some sense contrary to those seen in the existing literature, and are each discussed in turn. Structural Effects in the Experiments The structural findings can be accounted for by considering syntactic parallelism effects as arising from three distinct, co-occurring sources. Branigan et al. (1995) have suggested that parallel structure effects can arise when there is repetition of a single, high-level syntactic node, that is. a syntactic choice point in temporarily ambiguous structures. According to Branigan et al., coincidence of this choice point does not entail a node-for-node correspondence in syntactic subconstituents. The authors suggest that the effect is competitive; only when two alternative structures are competing to be assigned by the parser will the effect be found, and furthermore, not all ambiguities will Show the effect. Only those that cause significant initial parse difficulty or engender a difficult reanalysis will show benefits of repetitim. Some evidence for a single-node choice point component to parallelism benefit is found in the present experiments. In Experiment 1, larger second clause analysis regions show marginal NMA parallelism effects, and these reach significance in the second clause analysis that includes only the fully parallel MA sentences and their NMA counterparts. Of course, the NMA/NMA sentences share not only the single-node attachment site for the sentence complement. but also all subconstituent nodes. A single-node choice point contribution to syntactic priming is Unlikely to be the only source of effects in the NP objectIS-Complement ambiguity because it is found in some studies but not others, yet the structural choicepoint defines the ambiguity in all instances. Branigan et al. (1995) do not find parallelism benefit for either MA or NMA sentences in their experiment, perhaps because the NP objectIS-Complement ambiguity is relatively weak, as they suggest. and more susceptible to change as a result of lexical or semantic stimulus properties, a point which I will address below. Branigan et al. (1995) also discuss the possibility of a more global contributim to syntactic parallelism benefit. Here, in the extreme case, each syntactic node would have to be repeated for benefit to occur. Effects are likely in more moderate situations. however, with syntactic priming occurring for portims of sentences containing more limited node-for-node correspondence. Note that this source of parallelism benefit is not necessarily tied to structures that exhibit temporary structural ambiguities, but may be more general. in many parsing experiments (not only those intended to investigate parallel structure effects). the single-node choice point and node-for—node 57 similarity of an ambiguity are not independent of each other, that is, structures are relatively homogeneous. In the NMA structures presented here, a similar, single-node syntactic choice point is shared among items, and so is the node- for-node structure. In the MA sentences, though, there is a dissociatim in the single-node attachment site and the node-for-node structure. Node-for-node syntactic descriptions of the NP objects are quite different, though they retain the single-node NP object attachment site at a similar point in the sentence. if parallelism benefits are based solely on node-for-node coherence of syntactic descriptims of the first and second clauses, then MA parallelism effects should have been strongest in the full second clause analysis of fully parallel items and weakest in the full set of items, which is the converse of the results obtained in Experiment 1. I suggest that there is a third, semantic component to parallel structure effects: the lexical and conceptual similarity of the two clauses of a sentence. Subjectiveiy, it seems that stimuli used in previous research present a continuum along this dimensions. The present experiments use sentences with some overlap of lexical and cmCeptual structure, though not nearly as much as the original Frazier et al. (1984) study Upon which they were based. Branigan at al. (1995), m the other hand, made a concerted effort to reduce lexical and conceptual overlap even more than I have here. in terms of semantic (and syntactic) complexity, Frazier et al.'s stimuli are the least complex, Branigan et 58 al.'s lie in the middle, and the present stimuli (the MA sentences only) appear to be rather difficult. In the present experiments. the sensibility norming judgments can be seen as a metric for quantifying this semantic subcomponent of parallel structure effects. Sentences with MA first clauses and conjoined MA clause I; sentences were judged less sensible than sentences with NMA first Clauses or . conjoined NMA clause sentences. The NMA conjoined sentences may be : sensible in such a way that they do not show the same gain from repetition as in cases with more difficult adjectival and adverbial sequences. Evidence for L this account is seen in two results of the present experiments. If we assume that the fully parallel subset of MA sentences are also the least complex MA ' sentences (possibly because the more complex a syntactic structure is, the harder it is to create parallel clauses). then the lack of an NMA advantage in the first clause, fully parallel analysis of Experiment 1 is a result of equalizing the complexity of the two versions of the ambiguity. The correlational analyses of sensibility and processing benefit are more striking. At the disambiguating regim in Experiment 1, and at the main verb and disambiguating regim in Experiment 2, relative processing time benefit for a given MA parallel sentence is strmgly correlated with the sensibility benefit of that sentence. but there are no significant processing and sensibility benefit correlations for the NMA parallel sentences. 59 Both the correlational analyses of parallel sentence processing benefit and sensibility benefit, and the processing time results, show differences across the two tasks used in this study. MA parallelism effects are robust in both experiments, but NMA parallelism is only found in reading (and there mly marginally). Why should the tasks differ in their sensitivity to sensibility effects? The answer may lie in the size of the segments used in the AMW task. In Ferreira et al. (1996), participants rated how natural the task was for them. how difficult it was to comprehend segments, and how often difficult to understand segments became clear to them by the end of the sentence. Participants rated the task as fairly natural, and did occasionally have difficulty understanding segments. These difficulties were almost always resolved by the end of the sentence. First clauses in Experiment 2 were presented whole, and in MA clauses, subjects were faced with segmenting the speech stream of a complex object NP. Several short segments (adjectives, typically) are then paced through, one after another, in the second clause MA interchanging regim. it is likely that these short segments were less intelligible to subjects, placing different processing demands on subjects than when reading the same short segments in Experiment 1. The demands of the listening task may have made MA sentences even more difficult to comprehend than NMA sentences and inflating the parallel structure benefit observed. A more systematic investigation of how structural facilitation is moderated by structural difficulty is certainly warranted. Strong MA parallel 60 effects may be a result of global-level procedures that can be primed to attach a large number of similar structural nodes quickly when faced with complex . NPs. Other investigations have not used NPs as structurally complex as the mes presented here. and NP structure should be a manipulated variable in future studies. Prosodic Effects: Where are They? Several key questions remain regarding Iisteners' reliance on prosodic I“ It structural characteristics as cues to parsing. The environment of comprehenders is filled with informative (and perhaps not so informative) __~~ prosodic features, and both garden-path and constraint-based parsing models need to account for findings of prosodic effects in the literature. The present experiments were not designed to adjudicate between these two models of parsing - they were intended to examine syntactic parallelism effects in a new way. and to see if repeating a prosodic structure can provide the kind of benefit previously seen in the processing of repetitive syntactic structures. The acoustic analyses of the stimuli indicate that the structures used here were not realized differently by the speaker. Analyses were conducted on the first portions of the first clause, in regions where Beach (1991) showed effects of manipulations in off-line matching. Whether prosody can be used to guide an initial parse or to aid in reanalysis is still an open question, but it is possible that in a matching task like Beach's, effects are late in the stream of processing, in reanalysis of the presented fragment. While Ferreira et al. 61 (1996) have argued convincingly that the AMW is a sensitive, m-line technique. we should only expect to see effects if prosodic structural cues are actually present. Researchers have concluded that prosodic cues to a given structure are not perfectly predictive, but that a probabilistic relationship holds in the mapping of prosodic structure to syntactic structure. It is likely also that different temporary syntactic ambiguities vary in the extent to which they are prosodically realized by Speakers in natural contexts. and thus how dependable the prosodic characteristics would be for comprehenders to rely 9% on. The present results suggest that the NP objectIS-Complement ambiguity may be particularly variable in its prosodic cueing. Determining which structures are produced with strong and stable prosodic characteristics is an important endeavor for researchers in the field. We can then conduct experiments utilizing parametric manipulations of these characteristics if we hope to understand the relationship between the processing of prosody and syntactic structural assignment. APPENDICES APPENDIX A IT 62 APPENDIX A Experimental Sentences Jim believed Tom's $ stories were fantastic | fantastic new stories 5 and Sue believed Jim's 8 stories were fictitious | very boring stories 8. The explorer learned the route to the S Pacific Coast was difficult I very rainy Pacific Coast 8 and his guides learned the route to 8 South America was treacherous | very remote South America 8. Sally doubted the solution to the S physics problem was easy I really hard physics problem $ and Ervin doubted the solution to the 8 math problem was trivial | one difficult math problem 8. Johnny claimed the old basketball at the $ garage sale was deflated | very crowded garage sale $ and his mother claimed the 8 volleyball net was torn | torn old volleyball net 8. The newspaper admitted atrocities in $ Asia weren't yet over | war-stricken southeast Asia 3 and Amnesty international admitted increased torture of 8 prismers had just started I new and younger prisoners 8. Tim guessed the terrorists' 5 meeting place was hidden nearby | very well hidden meeting place 3 and the inspector guessed the Iocatim of their 8 computer network wouldn't be found I top-secret new computer network 8. Friends in Washington mentioned the news about 8 Clinton was true I young man Clintm S and other friends mentioned the rumors about 8 Nixon were true | old crook Nixon 8. The spy suspected the man with the S umbrella killed Harold j wonderful old umbrella S and the cop suspected the 8 woman witnessed everything | impatient female witness 8. Developers predicted the construction of the $ skyscraper would continue | new gigantic skyscraper $ and the bankers predicted the decay of 8 nearby neighborhoods would quicken | quiet old nearby neighborhoods 8. .- 1:." r3 -'-. t - 63 The skiers felt the S strong winds die down | fierce new strong winds S and the instructor felt the 8 dropping temperature stabilize | dangerous dropping temperature 8. The museum's tour guide denied the S girls should leave | bright young girls S and the security guard denied the 8 tour guide should hurry | busy new tour guide 8. The clever boy asserted the truth about the S Earth's age wasn't well understood j poorly-designed deep space probe S and he asserted the 8 I: implications were interesting I radical new implications 8. Bugs Bunny confessed the time of his S business meeting was changed I lmg- planned business meeting S and Daffy Duck confessed the stealing of the 8 briefcase was wrong | wrong black briefcase 8. l The instant replay revealed the call of the S referee was incorrect | most hated referee S and the league office revealed the 8 new technology was faulty I quite costly new technology 8. The woman maintained the S sculpture was truly beautiful | beautiful and modern sculpture S and the art dealer maintained the 8 client was wealthy | wealthy young client 8. The scientist implied the S theory was crazy | greatest new idea S and the lab assistant implied the previous 8 results were fabricated | year's undocumented results 8. People at last year's Rose Bowl noticed the S streaker was wearing tennis shoes I very thin and smiling streaker S and the announcers noticed the 8 quarterback was watching closely | well-conditioned young quarterback 8. Students in the advanced philosophy class observed the S teacher was unshaven | unshaven new teacher S and the teacher observed the 8 clock hands moved slowly I slowly-moving clock hands 8. The insurance agent inferred the S procedure's cost would rise | new procedure's high cost S and the patient inferer the 8 surgery would take months | long-term surgical risks 8. The plane crash survivors realized their S rescue wouldn't happen soon | quickly-fading rescue hopesS and journalists around the world realized the 8 story would be powerful I bold story's positive impact 8. 64 The prisoner asserted his S rights had been abused | own civil rights views S and the police department asserted its 8 innocence was now questioned | quite lmg-questimed innocence 8. The network president denied the S news story was fabricated | widely-reported news story S and the company spokesperson denied the 8 recent accusatims were serious | recent and serious accusatims 8. The young boy mentioned the S school fight had bothered him I cruel and vicious school fight S and the principal mentioned the 8 parents were very concerned | concerned and upset parents 8. Sherlock Holmes noticed the S man's shoes were muddy | men's muddied brown shoes S and Watson noticed the 8 suspect was quite upset | quite upset young suspect 8. in the sentences above, lexical material that is swiched to create first clause conditions is shown within dollar sign symbols, and is delimited by the piping symbol (|). The lexical material that is swiched to create second clause conditims is shown within ampersands, and is delimited by the same symbol. APPENDIX B «I- 65 APPENDIX B MAIN VERB SENTENCE COMPLEMENT I NOUN PHRASE PREFERENCE NORMATIVE JUDGMENTS 11 neutral verbs ' 9 biased verbs S-Completim I NP Completion / Other S-Completim I NP Completim I Other 1. infer 4315017 2. maintain 7912110 3. suspect 2917110 4. confess 43143114 5. admit 3615717 6. assert 5014317 7. deny 50/4317 8. doubt 3615717 9. mention 4315710 10. notice 50/5010 1 1. predict 5714310 . realize 7193/0 believe 21129150 . learn 64121115 claim 7171/22 .guess 43121136 .feel 36121143 . reveal 79/2110 . observe 8611410 . imply 2117910 toooxroaotgscogo-s average 4714914 average 40141/18 average of 6, 7, 9, 10 4814814 WEIGHTED AVERAGE FOR 11 NEUTRAL VERBS + 4 NEUTRAL VERBS + 9 NON-NEUTRAL VERBS: 45/46/9 APPENDIX C Table 1 Frazier et al. (1984) - Experimental Sentence Conditims Active/Passive 1a. The tall gangster hit John and the short thug hit Sam. 1b. John was hit by the tall gangster and Sam was hit by the short thug. 1c. The tall gangster hit John and Sam was hit by the short thug. 1d. John was hit by the tall gangster and the short thug hit Sam. Minimal Attachment/Nonminimal Attachment 2a. Jim believed all Tom's stories 2b. Jim believed all Tom's stories were literally true 2c. Jim believed all Tom's stories 2d. Jim believed all Tom's stories were literally true NonshiftedIShifted Heavy NP 3a. Mary wrote a long note about her predicament to her mother 3b. Mary wrote to her mother a long note about her predicament 30. Mary wrote a long note about her predicament to her mother 3d. Mary wrote to her mother a long note about her predicament and Sue believed Jim's stories. and Sue believed Jim's stories were fictitious. and Sue believed Jim's stories were fictitious. and Sue believed Jim's stories. and Sue wired a telegram requesting more money to her father. and Sue wired to her father a telegram requesting more money. and Sue wired to her father a telegram requesting more money. and Sue wired a telegram requesting more mmey to her father. 67 Table 2 Experimental Sentence Conditions 1) The CIA guessed the terrorists' “meeting place was hidden nearby“ and the FBI guessed the location of “their computer network wouldn't be found.“ (NMA/NMA) 2) The CIA guessed the terTm'sts' “meeting place was hidden nearby“ and the FBI guessed the location of “their top-secret new computer network.“ (NMA/MA) 3) The CIA guessed the terrorists' “very well hidden meeting place“ and the FBI guessed the location of “their computer network wouldn't be found.“ (MA/NMA) ." 'm- fly 4) The CIA guemd the terrorists' “very well hidden meeting place“ and the FBI guessed the location of “their top-secret new computer network.“ (MAIMA) First Clause Syntax/Prosody NMA/NMA NMA/MA MAINMA MAIMA 68 Table 3 Experimental Conditions - Experiment 2 Second First Clause Second Clause Syntax/Prosody Clause NMA NMA / NMA MA NMA NMA I MA MA NMA MA I NMA MA NMA MA / MA MA 69 Table 4 Correlations of Difference Time and Sensibility Judgments Main Verb Disambiguation lnterchanging Region Structure . NMA/MA n.s. n.s. 0.202 x2=7.746, p<.01 MAIMA 0.503 0.381 n.s x2=13.255, p<.01 12:7,151, p<.01 70 Table 5 Correlations of Difference Time and Sensibility Judgment Parallel Benefits Main Verb Disambiguation lnterchanging Region Structure MAMA 0.574 0.518 n.s. x2=8.593. p<.01 12:6.703, p<.01 APPENDIX D <_ (Most) Sensibility Judgment (Least) -> 71 Figure 1 Sensibility Judgments NMA/NMA MAINMA NMA/MA MAIMA Sentence Type Reading Time (ms) 72 Figure 2 Experiment 1 Reading Time Second Clause Disambiguating Region 800 750 — 700 - 650- 600-— 550— 50° / J“ “Ll-1.1 NMA/NMA MAINMA NMA/MA MAIMA Sentence Type Reading Time (ms) 73 Figure 3 Experiment 1 Reading Time Second Clause lnterchanging Region 2500 - 2000- / 50° / / .__-_-j_-__ NMA/NMA MAINMA NMA/MA MAIMA Sentence Type 74 Figure 4 Stimulus Sentence Construction - Experiment 2 (8a) The prismer assertedlhis rights had been abusedl oooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee ooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo oooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo ......................................................... ooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee Material within the solid boxes in Be and 8b was switched to create the NMA first clause structure I NMA first clause prosody I NMA second clause structure condition. Material within the solid boxes in 93 and 9b was switched to create the MA first clause structure / MA first clause prosody 1 MA second Clause structure condition. Material within the dashed box in 9a then replaced the dashed box in 8a to create the NMA first clause structure I NMA first clause prosody 1 MA second clause structure condition. Material within the dashed box in 8a replaced the dashed box in 93 to create the MA first clause structure I MA first clause prosody! NMA second clause structure condition. Mismatching prosody conditions were then constructed by replacing material within the solid box in 8a with material within the solid box in 9b. to create the MA first clause structural NMA first clause prosody 1 NMA second clause structure condition. Material within the solid box in 9a was replaced with material within the solid box in 8b, to create the NMA first clause structure I MA first clause prosody I MA second clause structure condition. Material within the dashed box in 9a then replaced material within the dashed box in 8a to create the MA first clause stnicture 1 NMA first clause prosody 1 MA second clause structure condition. Material within the dashed box in 8a then replaced material within the dashed box in 9a to create the MA first clause structure I NMA first clause prosody I NMA second clause stntcture condition. Difference Time (ms) 75 Figure 5 Experiment 2 Structural Parallelism Benefit Second Clause lnterchanging Region 2750 - MIA/MA MAIMA MIA/MA MAIMA First Clause Structure I Second Clause Structure LIST OF REFERENCES 76 LIST OF REFERENCES Beach, CM. (1988). The influence of higher level linguistic information on production of duration and pitch patterns at syntactic boundaries. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 75, 8(1). Beach, CM. (1991 ). The interpretation of prosodic patterns at points of syntactic structure ambiguity: Evidence for cue trading relations. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 644-663. Branigan, H., Pickering, M., 8. Stewart, A. (1995, March). Syntactic priming in language comprehension. Paper presented at the Eighth Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Tuscon, AZ. Cooper, W.E.. (1980). Syntactic-tophonetic coding. In B. Butterworth (Ed), Language production. Vol. 1: Speech and talk. (pp. 297-334). San Diego, CA Academic Press. Cutler, A (1976). Phoneme monitoring reaction time as a function of preceding intonation contour. Perception & Psychophysics, 20, 5560. Cutler, A, 8: Fodor, J.A. (1979). Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. Cognition, 7, 49-59. Ferreira, F. (1993). Creation of prosody in sentence production. Psychological Review, 100, 233-253. Ferreira, F., Anes, M.D., 8. Horine, MD. (1996). Exploring the use of prosody during language comprehension using the Auditory Moving VVIndow technique. Journal of Psycholinguistr‘c Research, 25, 273-290. Ferreira, F., Henderson, J.M., Anes, M.D., Weeks, P.A., Jr., 8. McFarlane, DK (1996). Effects of lexical frequency and syntactic complexity: Evidence from the Auditory Moving Window Paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 324-335. 77 Ferreira, F ., 8 Clifton, C. (1986). The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 75-87. Ferreira, F ., 8. Henderson, J.M. (1990). The use of verb information in syntactic parsing: A comparison of evidence from eye movements and word-by- word seIf-paced reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 555-568. Frazier, L. (1979). On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club. Frazier, L., & Rayner, K (1982). Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178-210. Frazier, L., Taft, L., Roeper, T., Clifton, C., & Ehrlich, K (1984). Parallel structure: A source of facilitation in sentence comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 12, 421-430. Grosjean, F. (1983). How long is the sentence? Prediction and prosody in the on-line processing of language. Linguistics, 21, 501-529. Kjelgaard, MM, 8 Speer, SR. (1993, March). Prosodic structure, syntactic structure, and lexical verb preferences in the comprehension of temporary syntactic ambiguity. Poster presented at the Sixth Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Amherst, MA. Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA MIT Press MacDonald, M., Perlmutter, N., & Seidenberg, MS. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101 ,676-703. MacDonald, M., & Schuster, S. (1994, March). Paper presented at the Seventh Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, New York, NY. Marslen-Wilson, W.D., Tyler, LK, Warren, P., Grenier, P., & Lee, CS. (1992). Prosodic effects in minimal attachment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 73-87. Mahler, J., 8. Carey, P. (1967). The role of surface and base structure in the processing of sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 335-338. Nespor, M., 8. Vogel, l. (1987). Prosodic phonology. Dordrect: Foris. 78 Pike, KL. (1945). The intonation of American English. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Price, P.J., Ostendorf, M., Shattuck-Hufnagle, S., & Fong, C. (1991). The use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90, 2956-2970. Shields, J.L., McHugh,A., 8 Martin, JG. (1974). Reaction times to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continous speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 250-255. Slowiaczek, ML. (1981 ). Prosodic units as language processing units. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Speer, S.R., Crowder, R.G., & Thomas, L.M. (1993). Prosodic structure and sentence recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 336-358. Speer, S.L., & Slowiaczek, ML. (1990). The influence of prosodic structure on comprehending sentences with temporary syntactic ambiguities. Unpublished manuscript. Trueswell, J., Tanenhaus, M., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528-553. Warren, P. (1985). The temporal organisation and perception of speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge, UK Watt, SM, 8 Murray, W.S. (1993, March). Complement sentence parsing in the auditory modality. Poster presented at the Sixth Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Amherst, MA