7 . . n ,‘l 11“ Ill . , a.d«rua...fi‘.fi5 3G .. 553$ . ‘ ‘ . 7 .Pr.z;.am:.i§n¢mwm.nt . . .29. . NH. .. r ..va. 1.. .1. f. 5 ... 2 um“: fl? .. . I. ”#4 m.33. . in”? d. ”a. New; .2 s .. .. =1 . flaw? ‘ .. V warm. smug, ,.. Q1. fir. .w. . 41..-? :..tUnsntle0.Vvhu. 3 fittvlluv V I 4 D “Vol ‘31? , 1 1:42. .3. . L. :rmtlavn. . fri‘lh'i. Ill: v. ‘ ’ Isl! , . , ‘ - ‘ - Win/«Aminflimflmmg Ilfliilfl'l‘lil‘l’ilHHWEIII‘ITIWISIWiiiliifiifiil THtooS 3 1293 01565 0314 LIBRARY“ Mlchigan State Universlty This is to certify that the thesis entitled ANDHOBYNY, PECEIVED PEJUDICE AND OUTNESS AMONG LESBIAN AND BISEXUAL WOMEN presented by -, Katari Kaylene Brown has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. (kgeehl Psychology 74;ij Major professor Date ”A! 3%??? 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE ll RETURN BOX to remove this checkom from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or betore dete due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU Is An Afflrmettvo Action/Equal Opportunity Inetttulon Wane-9.1 ANDROGYNY, PERCEIVED PREIUDICE AND OUTNESS AMONG LESBIAN AND BISEXU AL WOMEN By Katari Kaylene Brown A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1997 ABSTRACT ANDROGYNY, PERCEIVED PREIUDICE AND OUTNESS AMONG LESBIAN AND BISEXU AL WOMEN By Katari Kaylene Brown The more likely stigma is, the more likely a person will reveal a stigmatizing characteristic, but a person with greater resources for reacting strategically to prejudice is more likely to reveal such a characteristic. Research suggests that androgynous persons may have greater resources in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, ego development and ego strength. The Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) and measures of outness, perceived prejudice and relational satisfaction were completed by 345 lesbian and bisexual women. Subjects were classified as androgynous, masculine, feminine, or undifferentiated. The study predicted a negative correlation between perceived prejudice and outness, weakest for androgynous subjects, as well as greatest outness in public and greatest relational satisfaction for androgynous subjects. None of the predictions was observed. Androgyny appears related to greatest outness, but masculinity plays a larger role than femininity. Femininity appears related to intimacy satisfaction. Copyright by KATARI KAYLENE BROWN 1997 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my appreciation for the guidance and support of my committee members, Elaine Donelson, Joseph Reyher, and Andrew Barclay. I also am grateful for the assistance of Lynda Bennett, Carol Persad, Ralph Levine, and my spouse, John Sherwood. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES .................................................................................................. vii INTRODUCTION .................................................................................................. 1 CHAPTER 1 BACKGROUND AND RATIONALE ................................................................ 2 Stigma in Relationships ........................................................................... 2 The Androgynous Stereotype ................................................................. 5 Androgyny and Adaptability ................................................................... 5 Interpersonal Adaptability ........................................................... 10 Androgyny and Ego Development ............................................ 11 Androgyny Among Lesbian and Bisexual Women. .......................... 13 Study Overview and Hypotheses ........................................................... 14 CHAPTER 2 METHOD ................................................................................................................. 17 Subjects ........................................................................................................ 17 Materials ...................................................................................................... 21 Procedure ..................................................................................................... 27 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ........................................................................... 29 Post Hoc Explanations .............................................................................. 31 Additional Observations and Discussion ............................................. 36 Perceived Prejudice and Gender Role Orientation ................ 37 Comparison of Lesbian and Bisexual Women ....................... 38 Relational Factors and Other Variables .................................... 41 Perceived Prejudice Child Items ................................................ 43 Demographic Characteristics ...................................................... 44 Open-ended Responses ................................................................ 45 SUMMARY ............................................................................................................. 47 BIBLIOGRAPHY .................................................................................................... 62 V Table 1: Table 2: Table 3: Table 4: Table 5: Table 6: Table 7: Table 8: Table 9: Table 10: Table 11: Table 12: Table 13: Table 14: Table 15: Table 16: LIST OF TABLES Outness item and factor correlation matrix .................................... 58 Perceived Prejudice item and factor correlation matrix ............... 59 Relational satisfaction item and factor correlation matrix .......... 6O Outness / Perceived Prejudice correlations by group .................... 61 Outness Comparisons—Aggregate (t test) ...................................... 62 Outness Comparisons—Own Family (t test) .................................. 62 Outness Comparisons—Partner’s Family (t test) .......................... 62 Outness Comparisons—Public (t test) ............................................. 62 Prejudice Comparisons—Aggregate (t test) .................................... 63 Prejudice Comparisons— Own Family (t test) ............................ 63 Prejudice Comparisons—Partner’s Family (t test) ...................... 63 Prejudice Comparisons—Public (t test) ......................................... 63 Relational Comparisons—Aggregate Satisfaction (t test) .......... 64 Relational Comparisons—Intimacy Satisfaction (t test) ............ 64 Relational Comparisons—Sexual Satisfaction (t test) ................ 64 Relational Comparisons—Togetherness (t test) .......................... 64 vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Mean Aggregate Outness by Group ................................................. 65 Figure 2: Mean Aggregate Prejudice by Group .............................................. 65 Figure 3: Mean Outness in Own Family by Group ....................................... 66 Figure 4: Mean Prejudice in Own Family by Group ..................................... 66 Figure 5: Mean Outness in Partner’ 3 Family by Group ................................ 67 Figure 6: Mean Prejudice in Partner" 3 Family by Group .............................. 67 Figure 7: Mean Outness in Public by Group ................................................... 68 Figure 8: Mean Prejudice in Public by Group ................................................. 68 Figure 9: Mean Outness in Les-bi-gay Group ................................................. 69 Figure 10: Mean Prejudice in Les-bi-gay Group ............................................. 69 vii INTRODUCTION This study is concerned with lesbian and bisexual women, and the relationship of gender role orientation to outness, perceived prejudice and relational satisfaction. Lesbian women often are stereotyped negatively for being too masculine or androgynous, but greater androgyny may be an asset for dealing with prejudice. Social stigma theorists assert that the greater the likelihood of stigma, or prejudice, the smaller the likelihood that a person will reveal a potentially stigmatizing characteristic. However, a person with greater resources for reacting strategically to prejudice is more likely to reveal a potentially stigmatizing characteristic in the face of prejudice because the potential harm is seen as more manageable. This theory may be applied to lesbian and bisexual women, who have the choice of being "out” about their sexual orientation and risking potential prejudice. Androgyny research suggests that androgynous individuals, compared with masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated individuals, may have greater resources with which to react strategically to prejudice, in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, greater ego development and greater ego strength. Androgynous lesbian and bisexual women may report greater outness in the face of potential prejudice because they possess these greater resources. These same characteristics may allow androgynous women to experience greater relational satisfaction. 1 Chapter 1 BACKGROUND AND RATIONALE Stigma in Relationships Jones, Farina, Hastorf, Markus, Miller, Scott and French (1984), in their social stigma theory, examine the dynamics of ”marked” relationships, in which one of the parties possesses a characteristic that may serve as a basis for stigma, or prejudice. They point out that a person with the ability to conceal a potentially stigmatizing characteristic is in a special situation, in that they can choose to hide the characteristic in order to avoid potential prejudice. They report that the tendency for a person to conceal a characteristic that is the object of prejudice is inversely related to the risk of prejudice perceived by the person in a particular environment. In other words, the greater the risk of prejudice, the higher the likelihood of concealment. However, the greater a person’s resources — such as situational and interpersonal adaptability, ego development, and ego strength —— the greater the likelihood that he or she will be able to react strategically to prejudice and improve the outcome potential of the relationship (i.e., reduce the harmful consequences of prejudice). Jones et a1 point out that a person with greater resources for reacting strategically to prejudice is more likely to reveal a potentially stigmatizing characteristic in the face of prejudice because the potential harm is seen as 2 3 more manageable. Examples of adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice include the following: choosing a time and context in which the impact of revealing the characteristic will be minimal, using interpersonal skills to put others at ease and allay fears of the stigmatizing characteristic, deflecting others’ negative evaluations, discovering ways to educate others about misperceptions and stereotypes in a manner they will not reject, promoting negative evaluation of the prejudice itself, and redirecting attention to one’s strengths in ways that reduce the salience of the stigmatizing characteristic and highlight one’s commonalities with the prejudiced person. This researcher applied the theory of Jones et al to gay, lesbian, and bisexual populations, who in most situations can choose to hide or reveal their sexual orientation. One’s “outness,” or openness about one’s homosexual orientation, in a given environment will be related to how much prejudice against homosexuality one perceives or expects in that environment. Fear of revealing one's heterosexual orientation is not considered, because heterosexuality is the sanctioned norm and doesn't serve as the basis for stigma in American society. Were it not for anti-homosexual prejudice, all or most homosexuals probably would be as “out" about their sexual orientation as heterosexuals are. Homosexual and bisexual men and women who choose to reveal their orientation, or “come out,” will be more likely to do so in less threatening situations, such as among members of the gay community, trusted friends, and family members whose attitudes they have assessed for possible prejudice (Herek, 1993; Katchadourian, 1989). Coming out will be less likely in situations where the threat of prejudice is higher and the individual perceives signs of negative attitudes toward homosexuality. Such sentiments are commonly perceived in public, 4 educational and workplace settings (Herek, 1993; Page and Yee, 1985). This study is concerned with lesbian and bisexual women. According to Jones et a1, a lesbian or bisexual woman’s voluntary outness in a particular setting will be inversely related to the level of prejudice she perceives in that setting. However, if an individual has ample resources — in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, and ego strength — with which to deal with prejudice, she will be more likely to risk dealing with prejudice, and reveal a non-heterosexual orientation, because she will see the potential prejudice as more manageable. Research on how minorities deal with prejudice in various situations already have suggested that response flexibility — or possession of and ability to choose from an array of coping skills from situation to situation, rather than employing narrow response sets without adjusting for situational variables — helps black women (Lykes, 1983), black families (Barbarin, 1983), Chinese-Canadians (Dion, Dion and Pak, 1992), black, Japanese-American and Mexican-American adolescents (Phinney and Chavira, 1995) and deaf adults (T idball, 1990) to deal effectively with prejudice and stigmatization. Response flexibility may act as a salient behavioral resource for an individual attempting to react strategically to prejudice, since a variety of coping skills may be required across different situations in which prejudice might arise. Lykes (1983) asserted that no one strategy is equally effective in every situation, but that to cope effectively the person experiencing prejudice must choose between more direct, instrumental options and less direct, more compromising options, depending on factors such as minority composition and perceived prejudice of people in the environment, personal control in the given situation, and sources of identified difficulty. In other words, a person experiencing 5 prejudice should be flexible in choosing coping options most appropriate for particular situations. The Androgynous Stereotype A common stereotype of lesbians is that they deviate from the traditional female gender role in ways such as exhibiting fewer feminine characteristics, more masculine characteristics, and being more androgynous, or exhibiting a combination of feminine and masculine characteristics. These putative attributes of lesbian women are generally viewed as undesirable (Eliason, Donelan and Randall, 1992). This particular stereotype of lesbians does have some credence. Lesbian women do tend to differ from heterosexual women in gender role orientation. Although lesbian and heterosexual women rate similarly on femininity, lesbians rate higher on masculinity, and are more often “androgynous," or relatively high on both femininity and masculinity (Dancey, 1992; Mihalik, 1991; Oldham, 1982). This study was motivated by a desire to identify ways in which this negatively stereotyped difference may act as an asset for lesbian women. Specifically, greater androgyny among lesbian women, and possibly bisexual women, may be associated with greater ability to deal with anti-homosexual prejudice. Androgyny and Adaptability Bem (1974) first hypothesized that gender role orientation is related to behavioral flexibility across situations. A person who has developed a sex- typed — solely masculine or solely feminine -— internalized self-concept 6 might be consciously or unconsciously motivated to suppress (or be unmotivated to learn) behavior that is considered undesirable for his or her gender role in order to maintain a stable self-concept. A solely feminine or masculine gender role orientation thus could act to limit a person's range of behavioral options in a given situation. A person who does not differentially suppress or avoid masculine or feminine behavior, whose self-concept is not linked to a particular gender role, and who is able to employ masculine and feminine behavior equally, is termed androgynous. Bern put forth the idea that androgynous individuals, compared with masculine and feminine individuals, might be more likely to show gender role adaptability in different contexts, behaving in a situationally appropriate manner without regard for a given behavior’ 3 stereotype as more desirable for one sex or the other. Lobel, Gur, and Yerushalmi (1989), and Marsh and Byrne (1991) have linked gender role orientation to self-concept and differential suppression or expression of gender stereotyped behavior. The Bern Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) (Bem, 1974) is currently the most widely used gender role orientation measure, though classification methods have varied. The BSRI’s masculinity and femininity scales, respectively, appear to measure primarily instrumentality and expressivity (Donelson, 1995; Lazerson, 1981; Spence and Helmreich, 1981), also referred to as agency and communion (Donelson, 1995; Lazerson, 1981), and dominance and nurturance (Lazerson, 1981; Wiggins and Holzmuller, 1981). Masculinity, instrumentality, agency and dominance are associated with self-protection, self-assertion, competency and ambition. Femininity, expressivity, communion and nurturance are associated with group preservation, group harmony, cooperation and warmth. Persons who score high on both 7 dimensions of the BSRI are termed androgynous, and those who score low on both dimensions are called undifferentiated. Those who score high on femininity and low on masculinity are feminine, and those who score high on masculinity and low on femininity are masculine. The masculinity and feminity scales of the BSRI are generally considered representative of personality traits that are reasonably stable over time. Reliabilities for test and retest over a period of four weeks ranged from a=0.76 to a=0.91 (BSRI Manual, 1981). Little study has been done to assess the stability of these scores over longer periods. The BSRI has received some criticism. Locksley and Colten (1979) questioned the validity of using general sex stereotypes as individual personality measures, and Pedhazur and Tetenbaum (1979) called attention to the inconsistent social desirability of items. Bem subsequently developed the short form of the BSRI with items comparable in social desirability on both scales (Bem, 1979). Spence and Helmreich (1981) claimed there was a logical contradiction in Bem’s use of the BSRI to tap two orthogonal dimensions (masculinity and femininity) as well as the unidimensional construct of sex- typing (calculated using femininity minus masculinity). Bem (1981) disagreed, countering that the BSRI, like any instrument, can be used as a theoretical tool relating to any of the dimensions it measures. Since androgyny is theoretically related to greater behavioral flexibility, it may be related to the possession of the resources a person needs to employ adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice as described by Jones et al (1984). Further inspection of androgyny research reveals that androgynous individuals may in fact possess greater resources in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, higher ego development, and greater ego strength. In relevant research, androgynous subjects usually are compared with masculine subjects, feminine subjects, and sometimes with undifferentiated subjects. Bem asserted that androgynous persons possess a wider range of behavioral options, and tend to perform comparably on different tasks regardless of their association with gender stereotypes (Bem and Lewis, 1975). Bem and Lewis found that, in situations that pulled for stereotypically masculine and feminine responses, androgynous college students displayed masculine as well as feminine behaviors, whereas masculine and feminine students differentially displayed behaviors consistent with their gender role orientation, though the evidence was stronger for women than for men. In a study by Orlofsky and Windle (1978), androgynous college students performed well on masculine and feminine tasks, masculine and feminine students perfom1ed well on tasks congruent with their gender role orientation but poorly on opposite gender tasks, and undifferentiated students performed poorly on both sets of tasks. Ho's (1981) study of children’s problem-solving behavior found that the performance of androgynous subjects exceeded that of masculine and feminine subjects, but did not significantly differ from the performance of undifferentiated subjects. Lobel, Gur, and Yerushalmi (1989) found that masculine and feminine children displayed a preference for responding to questions associated with the opposite gender role orientation, whereas androgynous children did not respond to questions differentially. Heilbrun, with Pitman (197 9) and Mulqueen (1987), concluded that androgyny may serve as a liability at times if a person encounters difficulty choosing from their larger, more flexible array of response options. Some factors correlating with androgyny are similarly related to either 9 masculinity or femininity alone. In these cases, the question is raised whether the factor in question may be associated more often with one of the latter dimensions than with androgyny per se. Other variables actually have a stronger association with femininity or masculinity than with androgyny. Both androgyny and masculinity are linked to well-being (Pyke, 1985) and self-esteem (Antill and Cunningham, 1979; Kimlicka, Cross and Tarnai, 1983). But masculinity may be more responsible for this relationship, through correlates such as high self-monitoring, internal locus of control, self- confidence, and positive expectations of achievement and affiliation success (Lee and Schreurer, 1983). Androgyny and femininity alike are associated with children’s positive evaluation by teachers as prosocial (Piche and Piante, 1991) and with better global coping skills (Ellis and Range, 1988). However, Kleinke and Himichs (1983) found high femininity subjects fared best in adjusting to a novel college lifestyle and gaining social acceptance. Heilbrun and others (Heilbrun, 1981; Heilbrun and Schwartz, 1982; Heilbrun and Han, 1986; Lee and Schreurer, 1983) have found evidence suggesting androgyny may be an asset for women, but not for men, because women are able to take greater advantage of gender role flexibility through blending, or combining, masculine and feminine traits. Also, androgyny appears to be negatively related to stress symptoms in women, but to have no significant relationship to stress in men (Heilbrun and Han, 1986). Krausz, Kedem, Ta] and Amir (1992) found that masculinity, not androgyny, was most adaptive in dealing with work stress among male nurses. Cross sex typing (high masculinity and low femininity for women; high femininity and low masculinity for men) (Orlofsky and Windle, 1978), as well as undifferentiated gender role orientation (Alain and Lussier, 1988; Kimlicka et 10 al, 1983; Krausz et a1. 1992; Prager and Bailey, 1985; Pyke, 1985), tend to be maladaptive for both sexes. Though one must be careful to discern between correlates of masculinity or femininity and correlates of androgyny, we can hypothesize that androgynous individuals, especially women, may have greater resources in the form of behavioral options than do non-androgynous individuals. Androgynous lesbian and bisexual women may be better able to engage in strategic reactions to prejudice — such as choosing an appropriate time and context to reveal their orientation — as compared to their non-androgynous counterparts. Interpersonal Adaptability Androgyny appears to be correlated with greater flexibility and adaptability interpersonally. Babledelis (1978) found androgynous subjects were more flexible in choosing personal versus social orientations toward others, and had the most balance between their desire for independence and their desire for intimacy and affection. Borders and Fong (1984) reported that androgynous subjects performed equally well in role plays requiring stereotypically masculine or stereotypically feminine interpersonal responses, whereas non-androgynous subjects displayed deficits either in both situations, or in the opposite gender stereotyped response. They also reported an ous subjects demonstrated a greater capacity for intimacy. Wiggins and Holzmuller (1981) found androgynous subjects displayed more flexible interpersonal characteristics than masculine and feminine subjects. Alain and Lussier (1988) found that androgyny is associated with healthy 11 attachment (as opposed to over-attachment or under-attachment) and effective adjustment to divorce. In Safir, Peres, Lichtenstein, Hoch and Shepher’ s (1982) study of married couples, androgyny was associated with greater sexual satisfaction and freedom from sexual dysfunction. These conclusions suggest that androgynous individuals may have greater resources in the form of interpersonal skills than do non- androgynous individuals. Androgynous lesbian and bisexual women may be better able to engage in strategic reactions to prejudice —- such as using interpersonal skills to put others at ease and allay the fear others may harbor about homosexuality — as compared to their non-androgynous counterparts. Androgyny and Ego Development Androgyny appears to be related to greater ego development. Prager and Bailey (1985) and Waterman and Whitbourne (1982) found that androgynous individuals exhibited the most successful resolution of Erikson’s developmental crises, followed by masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated individuals, in that order. Waterman and Whitbourne found that masculine individuals did better than feminine individuals on resolution of Autonomy versus Shame and Initiative versus Guilt (associated with agentic qualities), and feminine individuals did better than masculine individuals on resolution of Intimacy versus Isolation (associated with communal qualifies). Androgynous individuals, however, demonstrated better resolution on all crises than did masculine and feminine individuals. Prager and Bailey (1985) also found androgynous individuals to rate highest on a measure of Loevinger’s ego developmental stages. Androgyny appears 12 to be associated with greater psychosocial and ego development. Androgyny also has been linked to greater ego strength. Pond (1984) found that androgynous individuals experienced low anxiety and high confidence in their ability to deal with stressful situations, compared to masculine individuals, who experienced high anxiety and high confidence, and feminine individuals, who experienced high anxiety and low confidence. Also, compared to masculine and feminine individuals, androgynous individuals show less discrepancy between how they perceive themselves and how they would they would like to be ideally. In other words, they have greater intrapersonal congruence between their real and ideal selves. Additionally, Pond found that androgynous individuals tend to demonstrate a more moderate, stable self-concept, in that they endorsed a broader, less rigid set of items on the Interpersonal Check List compared to masculine and feminine subjects. Androgynous individuals, with the combination of low anxiety and high confidence, greater intrapersonal congruence, and self- concept stability, have greater tolerance for evaluation by self or others. In other words, they have greater ego strength. The foregoing studies support the idea that" androgynous individuals may have greater resources in the form of higher ego development and ego strength than do non-androgynous individuals. Androgynous lesbian and bisexual women thus may be better able to engage in strategic reactions to prejudice such as deflecting others' potential or actual negative evaluations, as compared to their non-androgynous counterparts. 13 Androgyny Among Lesbian and Bisexual Women In summary, androgyny is related to adaptability and flexibility in choosing behavioral response options and in interpersonal situations, as well as to ego development and ego strength. In the area of behavioral flexibility, it may have more salient desirable implications for women than for men. In terms of the theory of Jones et a1 (1984), the androgynous lesbian or bisexual woman is likely to have greater resources with which to react strategically to prejudice, in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, higher ego development, and greater ego strength. She’s likely to exhibit greater outness about her orientation when faced with potential prejudice, compared to masculine, feminine, or undifferentiated women, because she will see the potential prejudice as being more manageable. Androgynous lesbian and bisexual women will hypothetically be more open about their orientation than masculine, feminine, or undifferentiated lesbians, given a similar level of perceived prejudice within a particular social context. As mentioned before, the greater interpersonal adaptability associated with androgyny may include a greater capacity for intimacy, healthier attachment, and greater sexual satisfaction. Furthermore, problem-solving flexibility (Ho, 1981) and effective anxiety coping (Pond, 1984) may act as assets in coping with relational discord. For these reasons, the same characteristics that may allow androgynous individuals to react strategically to prejudice also may allow them to experience higher relational satisfaction than feminine, masculine, or undifferentiated individuals. Although most research on androgyny has been done using 14 heterosexual subjects, there is nothing to indicate that the results are not generalizable to lesbian and bisexual women. Lesbian women differ little from heterosexual women on most variables. In fact, women — regardless of sexual orientation — are more similar than different in their attitudes toward intimacy (Blumstein and Schwartz, 1983; Duffy, 1985-86; Hurlbert and Apt, 1993; Iasenza, 1991; Peters and Cantrell, 1991; Schreurs, 1993) and sex (Blumstein and Schwartz, 1983; Clunis and Green, 1993; Loulan, 1983; Nichols, 1983; Schreurs, 1993), and on life history variables (Clunis and Green, 1993). Blumstein and Schwartz (1983) and Kurdek (1991) found that although heterosexual, lesbian, and gay male couples tend to differ in ways related to gender, conclusions drawn from research with heterosexual couples generally are applicable to same-sex couples with similar characteristics, and vice versa. One factor on which heterosexual and lesbian women do seem to differ is androgyny itself. On the BSRI, both lesbian and heterosexual women rate similarly high on femininity, but lesbians tend to rate higher on masculinity (Dancey,1992; Mihalik, 1991 ; Oldham, 1982). Thus, lesbians are more often androgynous. This characteristic of the lesbian population may be adaptive in its association with greater resources with which to cope with prejudice. Androgyny among bisexual women has received little attention. Study Overview and Hypotheses In this study, the short BSRI (to measure gender role orientation) and a questionnaire (to measure perceived levels of prejudice, outness in different areas of life, relational satisfaction, and demographic information) [see Appendix A] was administered to volunteer lesbian and bisexual female 15 subjects. Jones et a1 (1984) predict that a negative correlation will emerge between level of outness and level of perceived prejudice in different settings of subjects’ lives, such as family, community, school and work. Because androgyny may be associated with resources for adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice — in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, higher ego development, and greater ego strength -— this study predicts that androgyny will moderate the correlation between outness and perceived prejudice, and that the correlation will be weaker for androgynous subjects than for masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated subjects, resulting in androgynous women reporting higher levels of outness in the face of similar levels of perceived prejudice. Finally, androgynous women are predicted to report greater relational and sexual satisfaction. Hypotheses and predictions for this study are as follows: Hypothesis: A person will be less likely to reveal and more likely to conceal a characteristic, such as homosexual orientation, the higher the prejudice against that characteristic he or she perceives in a given environment. Prediction 1: There will be a negative correlation between level of perceived prejudice and level of outness. Hypothesis: Androgynous persons possess greater resources in the form of behavioral flexibility, interpersonal adaptability, higher ego development, and greater ego strength, than do persons with a masculine, feminine, or undifferentiated gender role orientation, will be better able to employ adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice, and will be less likely to conceal a characteristic in the face of relatively high perceived prejudice. 16 Prediction 2: The correlation between level of perceived prejudice and level of outness will be weakest for androgynous subjects compared to masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated subjects. Prediction 3: Androgynous subjects, compared to masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated subjects, will report on the average a higher level of outness in the broadest and most public areas on the questionnaire, where the highest likelihood of prejudice exists. Hypothesis: Androgynous persons, having greater behavioral flexibility, greater interpersonal adaptability, higher ego development, and greater ego strength, are more adaptable in intimate relationships, and will experience greater relationship and sexual satisfaction than persons with a masculine, feminine, or undifferentiated gender role orientation. Prediction 4: Androgynous subjects, compared to masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated subjects, will report greater relational and sexual satisfaction. Chapter 2 METHOD Subjects Subjects were solicited in the United States, Canada, Australia, and the United Kingdom through community and university lesbian-bisexual-gay organizations, les-bi-gay bookstores [see Appendix B for poster], newsletters and publications addressing les-bi-gay issues (see ad copy below), and through word-of-mouth referral. Postings were sent to about 265 internet les-bi-gay discussion conferences, organizations and contact persons (see posting copy below), including conferences specifically addressing minority lesbian and bisexual concerns and interests. The following advertising copy was used in newsletters and other paper publications: LESBIAN & BISEXUAL WOMEN NEEDED: for study on dealing with prejudice. To receive anonymous questionnaire(s), contact Katari Brown with your mailing address: (616) 781-9353 or brownka5@pilot.msu.edu or Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, USA Michigan State University sponsored. The following posting was sent to E-mail conferences, organizations or contact persons: Subject: Lesbian / Bisexual Women Study Message: I am currently conducting a questionnaire study on dealing with prejudice among lesbian and bisexual women. The questionnaires will be completely anonymous. If you are a lesbian or bisexual woman and are interested in participating in this study, please E-mail me your POSTAL address, and a questionnaire will be sent to you. If you think you or your organization can distribute some questionnaires, please let me know and I will 17 18 send you several. Please forward this message to any person or conference where it may be of interest. Thank you for your help! Katari Brown brownka5@pilot.msu.edu Department of Psychology Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824 USA (616) 781-9353 Subjects could request a questionnaire by E-mail, phone, or surface mail, or obtain a packet from stacks left at bookstores, les-bi-gay organizations, or local groups. The phone number listed on posters, advertisements and E- mail postings was linked to an answering machine, on which participants could leave their addresses or request further information. The following phone greeting was recorded on the answering machine for the advertised phone number: ”Thank you for your interest in our study. Leave your name and mailing address to receive an anonymous questionnaire packet, or, if you already have a questionnaire, you can indicate that you would like a results summary after the study. To receive a return call, leave your name, phone number, and times that you can be reached. Please speak your message clearly after the series of tones. Thank you for your call." Questionnaire packets were sent by surface mail and included a business reply envelope in which to return the completed questionnaire anonymously. The first page of the questionnaire packet [see Appendix A] stated that the study was anonymous, that participation was voluntary, and that a summary of the study results would be mailed to all participants who had provided addresses. Completed questionnaires were received from 348 people. If all 789 questionnaires that were sent actually reached individuals, this constitutes a response rate of around 44%. However, quesh'onnaires sent in quantity to be 19 given out at remote locations may not have been distributed completely, so the true response rate may be higher. Since questionnaires were returned anonymously, it was not possible to ascertain differential response rates on the basis of mode of solicitation. Three questionnaires were returned by individuals under eighteen years of age and were not included in the analyses, so the final sample consisted of 345 women. Classification as lesbian or bisexual was based on self-report, as Iasenza (1991) found self-definition to be a reliable indicator of sexual orientation. Of the subjects, 61% identified as lesbian and 39% as bisexual. Of the bisexual women, 47% identified a preference for female partners, 1 1% for male partners, and 41% indicated an equal or variable preference in partner gender. The mean age of the sample was 31 years (SD=10.14), with a range of 18 to 65 years. African-Americans, Latinas, and Asian-Americans were somewhat under-represented in the sample, though a variety of ethnicities was reported. Eighty-five percent of respondents reported their ethnicity as White or European descent, 3% as Hispanic or Latina, 3% as mixed ethnicity, 3% as Black or African descent, 2% as Mediterranean, 1% as Asian, and 1% as Native American. One subject identified as Pacific Islander and one as foreign national, and four subjects did not indicate ethnicity. Thirty-one of the fifty states were represented by at least one subject in the sample, although the central part of the country was under-represented. About 5% of respondents reported their residence to be in Michigan in the Lansing/ East Lansing area, 10% in other Michigan areas, 29% in the northeastern states, 16% in other midwestern states, 11% in southwestern states (all from California), 10% in southeastern states, 8% in central states, 2% in south central states (all Texas), and none in north central states. About 6% of 20 subjects reported living outside the United States (10 in Canada, 7 in Australia, 3 in the UK, and 2 in Germany). The sample was relatively well-educated. Three percent of subjects reported having a high school diploma, 31% some college, 5% an associate’s degree, 32% a bachelor’ s degree, and 29% a graduate degree. Most of the subjects reported being employed, and slightly more than half were students. Sixteen percent of the sample were not employed, 35% were employed part— time, and 47% were employed full-time or more. Two were retired and three gave no response. Of those who were employed, 49% reported receiving hourly wages, 37% indicated they receive a salary, and 9% reported working by contract. The remaining 4% indicated another form of compensation. Forty percent of subjects were students and 13% were part-time students. A range of income levels was represented. Of the American respondents, 35% reported an annual income under ten thousand dollars, 19% between ten and twenty thousand, 13% twenty to thirty thousand, 12% thirty to forty thousand, 6% forty to fifty thousand, and 9% over fifty thousand. Subjects reported a wide range of spiritual beliefs. Thirty percent of the sample indicated a religious affiliation of Protestant, Catholic, or other Christian, and 7% endorsed a Jewish affiliation. Twenty percent indicated a non-jeudeochristian affiliation, 43% claimed no religious affiliation, and 3% reported two or more religious affiliations. Most subjects were in a relationship when they completed the questionnaires. Seventy percent of subjects reported a current relationship (11 of these indicated more than one current relationship), and 38% reported cohabiting with their partners. Thirty-three couples returned their questionnaires in the same envelope. Fourteen percent of the women 21 indicated that they have a child or children, and 13% reported having partners with a child or children. About 30% of mothers and partners of mothers had children living at home. The women were assigned to groups according to gender role orientation on the basis of their responses on the BSRI. Thirty-nine percent were classified as androgynous, 27% as masculine, 13% as feminine, and 10% as undifferentiated. Eleven percent of the questionnaires were incomplete and couldn’t be scored for gender role orientation. Because women who are relatively open about their orientation are most accessible, a sample representative of all lesbian and bisexual women, including those who are closeted, was difficult to obtain. Women reporting lower outness were probably under-represented. The resulting reduction in range of the outness variable probably had an attenuating effect on the correlations calculated here. Sampling bias may have affected the outness variable, since Katchadourian (1989) reported that age, higher income, and greater education may be associated with greater outness. Materials Subjects completed anonymous questionnaires including the short BSRI. Questionnaire items [see Appendix A] included questions about demographics, relationship status and living situation. Three sets of rating scale items were scored: one on level of openness, or outness, about one’s orientation in various relationships and settings; one on perceived level of anti-homosexual prejudice in each of these areas; and one on relational and sexual satisfaction. The first two scales are intended to gauge a subject’s 22 perceived prejudice and outness in different interpersonal contexts. The second is meant to estimate a subject’ 3 level of satisfaction in several areas of a current or past relationship. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were conducted on each of the three original scales. On the outness scale, five factors were analyzed: (1) Outness with Children (a=0.765)(items: outness with your children, outness with your partner’ 5 children), (2) Outness with Own Family (a=0.820) (items: outness with your parents, outness with your siblings, outness with your extended family), (3) Outness with Partner's Family (a=0.852) (items: outness with your partner’s parents, outness with your partner’ 8 siblings, outness with your partner’ 5 extended family), (4) Outness with Les-bi-gay Group (a=0.562) (items: outness with les-bi-gay friends, outness with les-bi- gay community), and (5) Outness in Public (a=0.935) (items: outness with heterosexual friends, outness with local community/ neighbors, outness with acquaintances through church or religious groups, outness with coworkers, outness with business associates, outness with schoolmates, outness with professors/ teachers at school, outness with health care workers) [see Appendix C, Table 1]. The Outness with Children items were ambiguously related to other items and factors. This may have resulted from measurement error due to the small proportion of responses on these items (N =44 and N =35, respectively), or it may be that the relationship with one’s own and one’s partner’s children correlates uniquely with other variables. The Outness with Les-bi-gay Group factor is somewhat weak in reliability. This may be in part because anti-homosexual and anti-bisexual prejudice were not measured separately. 23 On the perceived prejudice scale, five factors along items parallel to the outness factors were analyzed. These were: (1) Prejudice from Children (a=0.593) (items: prejudice you perceive from your children, prejudice you perceive from your partner’ s children), (2) Prejudice in Own Family (a=0.706) (items: prejudice you perceive from your parents, prejudice you perceive from your siblings, prejudice you perceive in your extended family), (3) Prejudice with Partner’ 5 Family (a=0.789) (items: prejudice you perceive from your partner’ 5 parents, prejudice you perceive from your partner’ 5 siblings, prejudice you perceive in your partner’ 8 extended family), (4) Prejudice with Les-bi-gay Group (a=0.870) (items: prejudice you perceive from les-bi-gay friends, prejudice you perceive in les-bi-gay community), and (5) Prejudice in Public (a=0.837) (items: prejudice you perceive from heterosexual friends, prejudice you perceive in local community/ neighbors, prejudice you perceive from acquaintances through church or religious groups, prejudice you perceive from coworkers, prejudice you perceive from business associates, prejudice you perceive from schoolmates, prejudice you perceive from professors/ teachers at school, prejudice you perceive from health care workers) [see Appendix C, Table 3]. Consistent with the outness scale analysis, Prejudice from Children appears to have an ambiguous relationship with other items and factors. Prejudice in Les-bi-gay Groups, however, appears to have greater reliability compared to its counterpart in the outness scale. The other factors are slightly lower in reliability than their outness scale counterparts. Perceived prejudice is probably less dependent on the individual and more dependent on external variables, and therefore is likely to be less predictable than outness. 24 Some items were ambiguous. Four of the outness items (outness with partner's parents, outness with schoolmates, outness with teachers, outness with health professionals) loaded highly on the Outness with Children factor, perhaps because these items were ambiguous in not specifying, for example, ”your teachers” versus ”your children’s teachers.” On both the outness and perceived prejudice scales, the items pertaining to the subject’ 5 extended family and to church acquaintances loaded similarly on the family factors and the Public factors. Outness in these two areas may have a particularly strong relationship with outness in other areas of one’s life. For the primary analyses, the child and les-bi-gay factors were not included, since a minority of subjects had children, and the hypotheses did not specifically address the les- bi-gay area. However, an aggregate average was calculated using all outness items and all perceived prejudice items. The factors, Outness with Own Family, Prejudice in Own Family, Outness with Partner’ 5 Family, and Prejudice in Partner" 3 Family, Outness in Public, and Prejudice in Public, were not altered, since they display content validity and the hypotheses were not dependent on the internal consistency of the scales. The averages of the items from these factors were considered in the analyses along with the aggregate averages. Three factors emerged on the relational satisfaction scale. The factors were as follows: (1) Intimacy Satisfaction (a=0.861) (items: social intimacy with partner, intellectual intimacy with partner, recreational intimacy with partner, emotional intimacy with partner, level of affection expressed by partner toward you, level of affection expressed by you toward partner), (2) Sexual Satisfaction (a=0.848) (items: sexual intimacy with partner, quality of sexual contact, frequency of sexual contact), and (3) Togetherness 25 Satisfaction (a=0.678) (items: amount of time spent with partner, living situation) [see Appendix C, Table 3). The aggregate average of all relational satisfaction items was considered in the analysis, along with the average of the items from each of the three factors. The questionnaire had some inconsistent items. The questions concerning the subject’ 3 children and her partner" 3 children did not allow for indicating shared children [see Appendix A]. This wasn’t a problem in coding, since most subjects responding to both sets of items clarified their answers by writing notes on the questionnaire. The outness and prejudice scales did not separate anti-bisexual and anti-lesbian prejudice, nor indicate that anti-bisexual prejudice was to be included in ratings of ”anti- homosexual” prejudice. Some subjects noted this on their questionnaires. Several subjects rated the two kinds of prejudice separately; in these cases the mean of the ratings on a given item was used for analyses. The BSRI measures gender role orientation in terms of two continuous, statistically unrelated variables, masculinity (a=0.85) and femininity (a=0.84) (BSRI Manual, 1978). Bem (1975) developed the BSRI items [see Appendix A under ”Self-Description”] from characteristics rated by undergraduates as either more desirable (societally) for one gender than the other, or as equally desirable for females and males. Bem originally classified people as androgynous if the masculinity and femininity scores were similar, as masculine if the masculinity score was significantly higher than the femininity score, or as feminine if the femininity score was significantly higher, using hert ratio method to determine classification. The greater the difference between the M and F scales, the more sex-typed, or extreme, in 26 gender role orientation an individual would be. Spence, Helmreich and Stapp (1975) used their Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ) to classify people according to a median split method. Scoring above the median on both masculinity and femininity resulted in an androgynous classification, and scoring below the median on both resulted in an undifferentiated classification. People scoring above the femininity median and below the masculinity median were classified as feminine, and people scoring above the masculine median and below the feminine median were classified as masculine. Bem later adopted the median split method for the BSRI (Bern, 1979). Bern developed the short BSRI in response to criticisms that masculine items were more socially desirable than feminine items. The short BSRI masculinity and femininity scales correlate highly with measures of instrumentality and expressivity (Donelson, 1995; Lazerson, 1981 ; Spence and Helmreich, 1981). The median split scoring method was employed here, since it is the most widely used. In mixed gender samples, the BSRI Manual (1978) recommends calculating the medians from the sample. However, for a single gender sample such as this study’s female subjects, the normative sample medians are provided (F median-=55, M median=4.8). The difference score between F and M was examined as well, to explore for sex-typing effects. When it became clear during data collection that most subjects would not be recruited face to face, an open-ended question on personal experiences and coping strategies related to prejudice was added to the questionnaire [see Appendix A]. Seventy-one percent (246) of subjects wrote a response to this item. These data were not analyzed quantitatively because of time and resource constraints, but are addressed qualitatively in the discussion section. 27 Procedure Data collection took place from September of 1996 through February of 1997. Requests ranging from one to 25 questionnaires were received from 355 individuals. About 87% of requests were by E-mail, 11% by phone, and 3% by surface mail. Fifty-three respondents offered to distribute multiple questionnaires in their local community. Questionnaires were mailed to subjects along with business reply envelopes for anonymous return to the researcher. A total of 789 questionnaires were sent out — 317 individually or to a couple, and 472 to sixty-five different organizations or individuals who volunteered to distribute them locally, in quantities of three to twenty. A reminder of the deadline was mailed in early January by E-mail, or by a postcard which requested an E-mail address if applicable, since part of the E- mail database had been lost. The postcard contained the following copy: Thank you for your assistance with the MSU sponsored women’s study. I hope to receive all completed questionnaires by January 30, 1997 (This is a new date for some of you). I’ve received many already, and appreciate the time you’ve put in! Also, I unfortunately lost many of the Email addresses I had on record. If you have an Email account, please drop me a note at brownka5@pilot.msu.edu so that I can send future communications to you electronically. Thanks again for your help! Katari All communication with subjects took place through this researcher. A confidential database of names and addresses (when provided) was kept for the purpose of informing subjects about the study. Subjects’ identities were not linked to their responses on the questionnaires, nor to whether or not they had completed a questionnaire. Scoring of questionnaires — as well as entering, checking, and analyzing of data —— was performed by this researcher. 28 The statistical programs SPSS and CPA were used for data analysis. Confidence intervals were calculated for comparisons of correlations. The ttest for independent samples was used to compare means. Chapter 3 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Evidence was equivocal for all predictions. No predictions were supported as stated. Prediction 1: A negative correlation will emerge between level of perceived prejudice and level of outness. There was a negative correlation (r op-aggregate = -0.37, a=0.05) between the averages of all outness and perceived prejudice items [See Appendix C, Table 4]. This correlation was stronger on the Own Family factors (r op-ownfanfily = -0.46, a=0.05) and the Partner’ 3 Family factors (r oP-partnerfam = -0.44, a=0.05) than on the Public factors (r op-public = -0.35, a=0.05). Item by item, a significant negative correlation emerged in each area except in the lesbian-bisexual-gay community (r = 0.03, NS) and with the subject’ 5 children (r =0.02, NS). Prediction 2: The correlation between outness and prejudice will be weakest for androgynous subjects compared to masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated subjects. The androgynous group did show a weaker aggregate correlation (r OP-agsresate = -0.33, a=0.05) compared to the masculine group (r mm = -0.46, a=0.05), though the 95% confidence intervals overlapped 29 30 by .03, as well as compared to the feminine group (r opaggregate = —0.58, (1:005), but not compared to the undifferentiated group (r op—aggregate = -0.08, NS) [see Appendix C, Table 4]. On the Own Family and Partner’ 5 Family factors, the androgynous group showed the weakest correlation among the groups (r op-ownfamily = —0.36, a=0.05; r op—partnerfam = -O.28, a=0.05), though the 95% confidence intervals of the androgynous group and undifferentiated group overlapped by .03 for r op—ownfamily and .04 for r 0;).me [see Appendix C, Table 4]. Between the Public outness and prejudice factors, the correlation in the androgynous group (r op—public = -O.29, a=0.05) was weaker than in the masculine group (r op-public = -0.38, a=0.05), with an overlap in 95% confidence intervals of .08, and weaker than in the feminine group (r op-public = —O.71, a=0.05). However, the correlation was stronger compared to that of the undifferentiated group (r op-public = -0.04, NS) [see Appendix C, Table 4]. Prediction 3: Androgynous subjects, compared to masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated subjects, will report on the average a higher level of outness in the broadest and most public areas on the questionnaire, where the highest likelihood of prejudice exists. The average of the Outness in Public items reported by the androgynous group (M =3.98) was significantly higher (t (178) =3.64, (a=0.01) than in the feminine group (M =3.28), the undifferentiated group (M =3.16), or the non-androgynous groups altogether (M =3.52) [see Appendix C, Table 5]. However, the difference between the androgynous group and masculine group (M =3.78) was not significant. Prediction 4: Androgynous subjects, compared to masculine, feminine, 31 and undifferentiated subjects, will report greater relational and sexual satisfaction. The average of the Intimacy Satisfaction items was significantly higher (t (218) =2.27, a=0.05) in the androgynous group (M =4.08) compared to the masculine group (M =3.81), but the difference compared to the undifferentiated group (M =3.87) was not significant. The masculine group was also significantly lower (t (293) =2.17, a=0.05) on Intimacy Satisfaction compared to the non-masculine groups as a whole (M =4.05). The Intimacy Satisfaction value for the androgynous group and the feminine group (M =4.10) were nearly identical [see Appendix C, Table 14]. There were no significant differences among the groups on the aggregate average of relational satisfaction items, on the Sexual Satisfaction factor, or on the Togetherness Satisfaction factor [see Appendix C, Tables 13, 15 and 16 for mean values]. Post Hoc Explanations Prediction 1, based on the ideas of Jones et al (1984), lacked support only in two areas according to the outness item by prejudice item correlations. The hypothesis that a person will be less likely to reveal and more likely to conceal a characteristic the higher the prejudice against that characteristic he or she perceives in a given environment was not refuted, but may require rethinking. A negative correlation was not observed between outness and perceived prejudice on the lesbian-bisexual-gay community items. It’s possible that the items themselves were ambiguous. For example, some 32 subjects made note on the questionnaire that prejudice and outness in the lesbian-gay community versus the bisexual community should be considered separately, since anti-bisexual sentiment often is present in the lesbian-gay community but not in the bisexual community. However, when the correlation between the the les-bi-gay community outness and prejudice items was calculated separately for lesbian and bisexual subjects, there was no significant difference. Perhaps the stigmatized peer community simply constitutes an exception to the predicted negative correlation predicted by Jones et a1 (1984). In addition, the low variance (0.15) of outness in the Les-bi- gay Group may be responsible in part for the low magnitude of the outness- prejudice correlation. The correlation between outness and perceived prejudice on the items concerning les-bi-gay friends (r = -0.16, a=0.01) was significant in the expected direction. A negative correlation was not observed between outness with and perceived prejudice by the subject’ 8 children. The reason for this may be the difficulty of concealing of one’s orientation from children because of the intimacy and proximity of the parentst relationship. Children, unlike other groups, often live in the same household or visit on a regular basis. Frequent contact would increase the opportunities for employing adaptive strategic reactions to potential prejudice. Also, since children are of subordinate status, the parent has more control over the circumstances and consequences of revealing her orientation, and thus greater capacity for adaptive strategic reactions. Though Predictions 2 and 3 were not supported, the results were not inconsistent with the hypothesis that androgynous persons will be better able to employ adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice, and will be less likely to 33 conceal a characteristic in the face of relatively high perceived prejudice compared to other gender role orientation groups. The correlation between outness and prejudice for the androgynous group was consistently lower in each area except Outness in Public, where a nonsignificant correlation was observed in the undifferentiated group. Examining the size of the correlation between outness and prejudice may not have been the best way to test the hypothesis in question, since — despite the outness-prejudice correlations — the androgynous group reported significantly greater (t (169) =4.06, a=0.01) Outness in Public (M =3.98) than the undifferentiated group (M =3.16) [see Appendix C, Table 8] while the means of Prejudice in Public were not significantly different for the androgynous group (M =2.46) and the undifferentiated group (M =2.60) [see Appendix C, Table 12]. This suggests that the androgynous group will report greater outness in the face of similar levels of perceived prejudice, a conclusion consistent with androgynous persons possessing greater resources for adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice and thus having greater outness. Further, the androgynous group had the highest mean outness in all four areas considered (aggregate, Own Family, Partner’ 5 Family, Public) [see Appendix C, Tables 5—8], and there were relatively fewer significant differences in levels of perceived prejudice [see Appendix C, Tables 9—12]. Inspection of the outness and perceived prejudice means in graph form [see Appendix D, Figures 1-8] helps to illustrate this pattern. The androgynous group appears to report greater outness in the face of similar levels of perceived prejudice. Masculinity and femininity both correlated significantly with outness. Masculinity correlated with the aggregate average of outness ratings (r =0.32, a=0.01), Outness with Own Family (r =0.26, a=0.01), Outness with Partner’ 3 34 Family (r =0.28, (1:001) and Outness in Public (r =0.32, (1:001). Femininity correlated significantly with aggregate average outness (r =0.14, a=0.05) and Outness with Own Family (r =0.16, a=0.01). Femininity’s role appears to lie in the more intimate, less public areas of outness. Since the correlations are stronger and are significant in more areas for masculinity, and the androgynous and masculine groups differed in outness less from each other than from the other groups, masculinity probably plays a stronger role in outness than femininity. The self-esteem and positive expectations of success associated with masculinity may contribute to the ability to react strategically to prejudice in such ways as deflecting others’ negative evaluations or redirecting attention to one’s strengths. Femininity’s association with outness seems to be more limited, but nonetheless salient. It is notable that the feminine group scored higher, though not significantly, on aggregate outness (M =3.53), Outness with Own Family (M =3.01), and Outness in Public (M =3.28), than the undifferentiated group (M =3.36, M =2.47, and M =3.16, respectively), while scoring similarly on Outness with Partner’s Family. The social affiliation and social adjustment skills associated with femininity may contribute to the ability to strategically react to prejudice in such ways as highlighting one’s commonalities with the prejudiced person, or using interpersonal skills to put others at ease and allay fears about one’s sexual orientation. It may be a combination of femininity with masculinity that is responsible for the slightly larger, though insignificant, differences between the androgynous and masculine groups in outness [see Appendix C, Table 5—8] compared to the differences in perceived prejudice [see Appendix C, 35 Tables 9-12]. In a larger sample, this difference might reach significance and show androgynous individuals to have greater outness in the face of similar levels of perceived prejudice. Masculinity and femininity both appear to play a role in outness, with masculinity’s role greater and broader than that of femininity. Androgyny does appear to be associated with greater outness in the face of prejudice, but further study is warranted to clarify the specific roles of masculinity and femininity as well as their combined role. The results did not support Prediction 4, that androgynous subjects would report greater relational and sexual satisfaction than the other groups. Instead, it appears that femininity may be the salient variable associated with the Intimacy Satisfaction factor, while gender role orientation did not seem to be associated with the Sexual Satisfaction factor or the Togetherness Satisfaction factor [see Appendix C, Tables 15 and 16]. The feminine group was similar on the mean of the Intimacy Satisfaction items (M =4.10) compared to the androgynous group (M =4.08) [see Appendix C, Table 14]. The differences from other groups did not reach significance for the feminine group, possibly due to the feminine group’s smaller size (N =44) compared to the androgynous group (N =132). Femininity was correlated significantly with Intimacy Satisfaction (r =0.14, a=0.05) whereas masculinity was not. These findings are consistent with previous research that identified femininity as a correlate of social skills (Ho, 1981 ; Kleinke and Himichs, 1983; Piche and Piante, 1991) and the traits of expressivity, communion and nurturance . Neither masculinity nor femininity correlated significantly with Sexual Satisfaction or Togetherness Satisfaction. 36 Additional Observations and Discussion The size of the groups may have had an attenuating effect on the correlations and significance levels calculated here. The androgynous group (N =136) was larger than the masculine (N =91), feminine (N =44) and undifferentiated (N =35) groups. Uneven sample sizes are normal when classifying samples using the BSRI, and in female samples the androgynous group tends to represent the largest proportion (BSRI Manual, 1981). A relatively larger total sample size is required to attain significance in analyses concerning the masculine, feminine and undifferentiated groups. The self- report nature of the data gathered limits this study’ s ability to fully test the hypotheses put forth. Further investigation using more varied methods would help determine the hypotheses’ utility. Since subjects were solicited over the internet, the above average educational level (30% of subjects had a graduate degree) is not surprising. Though many persons without access to the internet probably were reached through the manual distribution of questionnaires by volunteer subjects and organizations, internet users were likely over-represented. Individuals who are less out or less involved with les-bi-gay organizations and media were not likely to be reached by this study’ s solicitation. The results are probably most generalizable to lesbian and bisexual women who are out in the les-bi-gay community or who are consumers of les-bi-gay literature, and who have the time and motivation to participate in a voluntary study. The difference score of the raw BSRI scales, femininity minus 37 masculinity (F-M), correlated negatively with the aggregate average outness (r = -0.16), with Outness in Public (r = -O.16) and with average outness in families (r = -.14). The F-M score did not correlate significantly with the prejudice measures. It appears that the larger a person’s F-M score, Bem’s (1981) original measure of degree of sex-typing, the lower outness they may be likely to report. Perceived Prejudice and Gender Role Orientation The differences among the groups in perceived prejudice are notable. The androgynous group reported significantly less (t (168) =2.60, a=0.01) perceived prejudice in Own Family (M =2.65) compared to the undifferentiated group (M =3.17) and the non-androgynous groups as a whole (M =2.93;t (300) =2.32, a=0.05) [see Appendix C, Table 10]. On Prejudice in Public, the mean for the feminine group (M =2.78) was significantly higher (t (177) =2.23, a=0.05) compared to the means for the androgynous group (M =2.46), the masculine group (M =2.46), and the non-feminine groups as a whole (M =2.48) [see Appendix C, Table 12]. The androgynous group reported consistently lower perceived prejudice, though the aggregate mean prejudice was the same for the androgynous and masculine groups (M =2.39). The feminine and the undifferentiated groups reported consistently higher perceived prejudice compared to the androgynous and masculine groups, though most of the differences were insignificant [see Appendix C, Tables 9-12]. Masculinity may play the principle role in perceived prejudice 38 differences. Masculinity correlated negatively with the aggregate average of perceived prejudice (r = -0.15, a=0.01), Prejudice in Own Family (r = -0.14, a=0.05), and Prejudice in Public (r = —0.16, (1:001), but did not correlate significantly with Prejudice in Partner’ 3 Family. Femininity correlated negatively with Prejudice in Own Family (r = -O.12, (1:005), but not significantly with the other prejudice variables. Masculinity may account for more of the differences in perceived prejudice among the groups, since it correlates significantly with prejudice in more areas, although femininity may play a role particularly in perceived prejudice in the subject’s own family. Comparison of Lesbian and Bisexual Women Bisexual individuals are studied less often than lesbian-gay individuals, and often are not considered separately in studies of people in same-sex relationships. When research does concern bisexual populations, the degree of preference for partner gender is seldom considered. This study offered the opportunity not only to compare lesbian and bisexual women, but to draw comparisons among bisexual women of varying preferences regarding partner gender. The subjects identifying as bisexual were similar to one another on most of the outness, perceived prejudice, and relational satisfaction variables. The women who identified as bisexual with an equal preference for partner gender rated significantly higher (t (57) =2.16, a=0.05) on the average perceived prejudice among the Les-bi-gay Group (M =2.23) compared to 39 women who identified as lesbian (M =1.41), bisexual with a preference for females (M =1.54), or bisexual with a preference for males (M =1.63). This may be due to the ambiguous position of bisexuals in the les-bi-gay community. In their open-ended responses, many women commented that ”biphobia," or prejudice against bisexuals, is often present in the lesbian and gay community. Bisexual women with a preference for female partners may be able to fit in as ”mostly” lesbian, and bisexual women with a preference for male partners may spend less time in the les-bi—gay community and thus have less exposure to and less awareness of anti-bisexual prejudice. Bisexual women with an equal preference for partner gender may be more visibly bisexual since there’s a greater likelihood that they will have relationships with members of both sexes throughout their lives. This visibility may raise the likelihood of encountering existing anti-bisexual prejudice, thus raising the amount of prejudice they perceive. Some examples of anti-bisexual prejudice subjects reported experiencing in the lesbian and gay community were: being told, ”You pass as a straight so you can take advantage of heterosexual privilege”; encountering a ”rule” among lesbian women of not getting involved with bisexuals; being told, ”You’re afraid to leave your marriage because you’re oppressed emotionally and socially”; being ”tagged as a traitor” by the lesbian community for dating a bisexual woman; being told, ”You bring the risk of AIDS to lesbian women”; being accused of ”sleeping with the enemy [heterosexuals]” ; and being excluded from lesbian-gay political and other organizations. Aside from perceived prejudice in the les-bi-gay community, bisexual women with different preferences for partner gender didn’t differ 40 significantly from each other, or in significantly different ways from lesbian women. Therefore, comparisons with lesbian subjects will consider bisexual subjects all together. Lesbian participants had higher (t (335) =2.73, a=0.01) femininity scores (M 6.74) compared to bisexual participants (M =5.51). There was no significant difference between the mean masculinity scores for the two groups. Accordingly, a slightly higher proportion (7%) of lesbian women was classified as androgynous, and a slightly lower proportion (9%) as masculine. Lesbian women reported significantly greater (t (343) =7.37, a=0.01) aggregate average outness (Xaggregate=4.11) than bisexual women (XmfiAl), and scored significantly higher on all outness factors. This may be due to the greater likelihood that bisexuals can appear heterosexual, while lesbians may have greater difficulty concealing their orientation. Bisexual women reported significantly greater (t (304) =3.91, u=0.05) aggregate average perceived prejudice (XaggregaFZ.61) than lesbian women (Xaggmgate=2.34), and scored significantly higher on all prejudice factors except Prejudice from Partner’s Family, where the difference did not reach significance. This may be a combined result of experiencing prejudice in both heterosexual and lesbian-gay areas. Lesbian women rated significantly higher (t (330) =3.40, (11:00]) on the Intimacy Satisfaction factor (M =4.09) than bisexual women (M =3.76). Since lesbian participants rated higher on femininity, this difference is consistent with the correlation between femininity and Intimacy Satisfaction. Since heterosexual women were found to be similar to lesbian women on femininity but lower on masculinity (Dancey, 1992; Mihalik, 1991 ; Oldham, 41 1982), an examination of masculinity and femininity in a sample combining lesbian, bisexual and heterosexual women may be of interest. Lesbian women also had a significantly higher (I (327) =3.07, a=0.01) mean Sexual Satisfaction (M =3.79) than bisexual subjects (M =3.40). Relational Factors and Other Variables Femininity appears to play a role in satisfaction with relational intimacy. However, it accounts for only a portion of the variance of one factor identified in this study. Other variables measured here may be playing a part as well. The association of the relational satisfaction factors with outness, perceived prejudice and relationship characteristics was explored. Outness and prejudice were associated with relational satisfaction factors. Intimacy Satisfaction was correlated with aggregate outness (r =0.14, a=0.01), Outness with Own Family (r =0.14, a=0.05), Outness in Public (r =0.18, a=0.01), and Outness with Les-bi-gay Group (r =0.13, a=0.05). Togetherness Satisfaction was related to aggregate outness (r =0.11, a=0.05) and Outness with Own Family (r =0.17, a=0.01). This relationship may result from greater freedom to express affection and appear as a couple associated with greater outness. Intimacy Satisfaction was negatively correlated with aggregate perceived prejudice (r = 0.12, a=0.05) and Prejudice by Partner" 5 Family (r = -0.14, a=0.05). Togetherness Satisfaction was negatively correlated with aggregate perceived prejudice (r = —0.17, a=0.01), Prejudice by Partner’ 5 Family (r = -0.14, a=0.05). Prejudice in one’s partner’s family, particularly for 42 same sex partners, may be associated with exclusion from family events, and more frequent separation from one’s partner when she spends time with her family. Sexual Satisfaction was negatively correlated with aggregate perceived prejudice (r = -0.11, a=0.05) and Prejudice in Public (r = -0.13, a=0.05). The association of Sexual Satisfaction and Intimacy Satisfaction with prejudice may be related to internalization of external prejudice (Brown, 1986) — in other words, experiencing shame and self-reproach for one’s sexual orientation which could interfere with enjoying sex and intimacy with a partner of the same sex. If one prefers a partner of the same sex, such internalization of prejudice may cause one to avoid relationships with persons of one’s preferred gender in order to escape negative selfoevaluation, even though relationships with an opposite sex partner may not be satisfying. Since bisexual women perceive greater levels of prejudice than lesbian women, including anti-bisexual prejudice in the les-bi-gay community, they may have greater levels of internalized prejudice. This would help explain the lower Intimacy Satisfaction and Sexual Satisfaction among bisexual women. The degree of exclusivity, or monogamy, in women’s relationships was associated with the relational factors. Greater exclusivity was correlated with Intimacy Satisfaction (r =0.29, a=0.05) and Sexual Satisfaction (r =0.20, a=0.05). Subjects reported greater satisfaction with intimacy and sex the more exclusive their commitment agreement with one another. Women who live with their partners rated higher outness to their own family (t (153) =2.72, a=0.01) and to their partners’ families (t (212) =4.33, a=0.01). The higher ratings of perceived prejudice in their own families by 43 women who don’t cohabit (t (231) =2.14, a=0.05) may be a reason for choosing not to cohabit and may or may not reflect accurate perceptions. The lower rating of prejudice by women who do cohabit may be due to these women’s influence on their family’s prejudice, or possibly to an intrapsychic need to believe their families are more supportive than they actually are. Women who reported cohabiting rated higher than non-cohabitors on the Togetherness Satisfaction factor (t (199) =5.53, a=0.01) but lower on the Sexual Satisfaction factor (t (233) =2.48, a=0.05). The difference in Sexual Satisfaction may be related to the connection of cohabitation with longer duration of the relationship, and the associated decline in sexual frequency in couples (Blumstein and Schwartz, 1983). It is notable that Intimacy Satisfaction was not significantly different for the cohabiting versus the non- cohabiting women, a result consistent with Blumstein and Schwartz’s finding that relationship satisfaction tends to be less associated with sex the longer a couple is together. Subjects with children or whose partners had children reported significantly lower (t (327) =2.24, a=0.05) Intimacy Satisfaction (M =3.76) compared to subjects without children (M =4.02). The time and effort parenting requires may at times limit efforts toward building intimacy with a partner. This is consistent with reports by heterosexual couples of reduced relational intimacy after having children (Blumstein and Schwartz, 1983). Perceived Prejudice Child Items As noted earlier, inter-correlations of related self-report scales are 44 usually in the positive direction. The negative correlations in the perceived prejudice item correlation matrix [see Appendix C, Table 2] were not expected and are worthy of attention. Only one of these correlations — prejudice by subject’ 5 children with prejudice by partner’s siblings (r = -0.43) — was significant (a=0.05). Some of the correlations may have failed to reach significance because of the small number of subjects with children and a partner (N =47), and many unusable ”not applicable” answers. The negative correlations may reflect a unique pattern of perceiving one’s children’s attitudes toward oneself and one’s attributes. If one perceives a greater amount of prejudice in areas of life outside the home, one may have an intrapsychic need to perceive a lower amount of prejudice by one’s most immediate family, such as children and partner. An alternative explanation may be that higher perceived prejudice outside one’s immediate family, viewed in comparison, causes prejudice within the immediate family to appear greater, and vice versa. The reasons for the negative correlations of the child prejudice items with other perceived prejudice items merit further investigation. Demographic Characteristics Katchadourian (1989) reported that such characteristics as age, income and education may be correlated with outness. Demographic characteristics may be correlated with other variables as well. Outness, as well as perceived prejudice, masculinity and feminity were examined in relation to demographic variables to explore for possible associations. Older participants generally reported greater outness. Age correlated 45 with aggregate average outness (r =0.20, a=0.01), Outness with Own Family (r =0.31, a=0.01), outness with Partner’ s Family (r =0.20, a=0.01), and Outness in Public (r =0.11, a=0.05). It appears that lesbian and bisexual women may become more open about their sexual orientation as they get older. Age did not correlate significantly with perceived prejudice variables. In comparing subjects who identified as white with subjects who identified with a minority ethnicity, only one significant difference was found. The mean femininity score for white women (M =5.69) was significantly higher (t (331) =2.41, a=0.05) than for non-white women (M =5.41). Although a minority (25%) of the non-white group identified as African-American, this difference is consistent with previous findings that African-American women adhere less to conventional standards of femininity than white women (Donelson, 1995). The difference in the mean feminity score for white and non-white women may be due to differing sanctions for and definitions of feminine behavior among ethnic subcultures, or differential sanctioning in the broader society of feminine behavior according to ethnicity. Open-ended Responses Participants who responded to the open-ended question on experiences of and strategies for dealing with prejudice wrote answers varying in length from one line to two typed pages. An equal proportion (about 75%) of subjects from each gender role group provided a response. Some examples listed by subjects as strategies that work for dealing with prejudice were: 46 trying to get the prejudice person to imagine him or herself experiencing discrimination, avoiding prejudiced people, being honest, getting to know people and selecting whom to come out with, using peer pressure, and withdrawing or ignoring prejudice if one is in danger. Some examples given by subjects as strategies that don’t work were: being aggressive or overtly angry, trying to force one’s views on others, and confronting prejudice with potentially aggressive people. The open-ended responses were not analyzed quantitatively. Future inquiry might be directed toward comparing the amount and types of experiences and strategies listed by subjects from different gender role orientations. Such analyses might reveal specific information about adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice relative to gender role orientation. SUMMARY Though none of the predictions was observed as expected, some of the hypotheses did receive support. A significant negative correlation between outness and prejudice was observed in most areas, but not for the items concerning the les-bi-gay community or the subject’ 3 children. The outness- prejudice correlation was not consistently weaker for androgynous subjects. Androgynous subjects did not report significantly higher public outness than masculine groups, but androgynous subjects did report consistently higher outness and consistently similar prejudice levels compared to masculine, feminine and undifferentiated subjects. Both masculinity and feminity were associated with outness. Masculinity was correlated more highly with outness than femininity, and in a broader range of areas, whereas femininity was correlated more weakly with outness and more specifically in family relationships. The androgynous group was not highest on relational satisfaction factors, but was comparable to the feminine group. The Sexual Satisfaction factor was not associated with gender role group. Femininity, however, correlated significantly with the Intimacy Satisfaction factor. Other characteristics of the data also were explored. Uneven group sizes, the self-report nature of the data, and self-selection of the sample should temper interpretation of the results. The femininity minus masculinity score (F-M) correlated negatively with outness. 47 48 Masculinity and femininity correlated in similar magnitude with prejudice, but femininity’s role was specific to family relationships. Bisexual women indicating an equal preference for partner gender rated highest on the Prejudice in Les-bi-gay Group factor. Lesbian subjects rated higher on feminity, and were more often androgynous. Bisexual women reported greater levels of prejudice, and lesbian women reported greater Intimacy Satisfaction and Sexual Satisfaction. Subjects’ reports of anti-bisexual prejudice in the lesbian and gay community merit further investigation. Outness was associated with Intimacy Satisfaction and Togetherness Satisfaction. Prejudice was correlated negatively with all three relational factors. The possible relationship of perceived prejudice and internalized prejudice should be explored. Greater relationship exclusivity was associated with Intimacy Satisfaction and Sexual Satisfaction. Cohabiting women reported greater outness and lower perceived prejudice than non-cohabiting women. Cohabiting women rated higher on Togetherness Satisfaction but lower on Sexual Satisfaction. Subjects with children reported lower Intimacy Satisfaction. The items concerning prejudice by children were correlated negatively with some other items; while only one correlation was significant, this pattern merits further study. Age was positively correlated with level of outness. White subjects rated higher on femininity compared to non-white subjects. Further qualitative and quantitative analysis of the open-ended responses would be useful to explore for differences among gender role groups in adaptive strategic reactions to prejudice. APPENDICES APPENDIX A APPENDIX A QUESTIONNAIRE FOR SUBJECTS Katari K. Brown Department of Psychology Michigan State University 135 Snyder Hall East Lansing, MI 48824 USA (616) 781 -9353 brownka5@pilot.msu.edu Dear Research Participant, Thank you for your interest in our study on dealing with prejudice among lesbian and bisexual women. This questionnaire takes 12 to 20 minutes to till out. By completing and returning the questionnaire, you are indicating your voluntary agreement to participate. Your assistance is much appreciated! Please separate this cover letter from the questionnaire and retain it tor your intomiation. Then. fill out the questionnaire, answering as best as you can from your own knowledge and experience. and mail the completed questionnaire in the envelope included in this packet. To preserve your anonymity, do not write your name on the questionnaire. it you are in a relationship, and both you and your partner participate, please return both your questionnaires in the same envelope. We hope to receive all questionnaires by January 30, 1997. A summaryoitheresultstrornthestudywill be mailedtoall partic‘pantswhoseaddresses we have. It you didn't receive your questionnaire by mail, and would like to receive the summary, please call the above number, give your address, and indicate that you’d like a summary of the study results. It you decide not to participate in the survey, ieel tree to pass on this questionnaire to a friend who may be interested. Alter January. please destroy any unused questionnaires. Again, your time and participation are much appreciated. Yours Sincerely. fl” Katari K. Brown 49 50 APPENDIX A Subject Nur'rber Questionnaire for Subjects Demraghic lnformtion: i am female, and my sexual orientation is (check one): lesbian bisexual, prefer female partners bisexual, prefer male partners bisexual, equal preference Age in years Ethnicity (check one): Black Native American Mediterranean Hispanic Asian White Other (specify) Place of residence (check one) Lansing I East Lansing area Kalamazoo area Battle Creek area Ann Arbor area ____Other (specify city and state) Education (check one) __less than high school high school d'ploma some college __associate’s degree bachelor’s degree graduate degree Are you ermloyed? (check one) No Yes, part-time Yes, full-time or more If yes, how are you paid? (check the one that best applies to your main occupation) _hourly wages __salary _commission _contract Are you a student? (check one) No Yes, part-time Yes, full-time \M'iich option best describes your personal annual income level, in dollars? (check one) less than 10,000 10,000 to 19,999 20,000 to 29,999 30,000 to 39,999 40,000 to 49,999 50,000 to 59.999 60,000 to 69,999 70,000 or more Which option best descrbes your religious affiliation? (check one) Fundamental Protestant Conservative Protestant Liberal Protestant Conservative Catholic Liberal Catholic Mormon Liberal Jewish Orthodox Jewish None ___Other (specify) 51 APPENDIX A W91: Which best describes your involvement in lesbian I bisexual / gay organization(s) and / or support groups? (check one) __not involved _somewhat involved __actively involved __hold or have held formal position or office How many children do you have? if more than zero, number under 18 years of age If more than zero, number that live in your household Which best describes where you live right now? (check one) own house or condominium __rent house _rent apartment _rent room only rent dorm room other (specify) Which best describes your current living situation? live alone live with relative(s) live with room I housemates cohabit with partner (Indicate length of cohabitation: __wars __months) 52 APPENDIX A utness: indicate on a scale from 1 to 5 your openness about your sexual orientation with each of the below groups using this rating scale: in the given group, you are 1-openwithnoone 2-openwithoneortwopeople OR muchconcealment 3-open with some people on some concealment 4-openwithmostpeople OR rnoderatelyopen 5-openwitheveryone OR veryopen NA - not applicable, nocontact with this group (circle one nunber or 'NA' for each item) NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your children NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner’s children NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your parents NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner's parents NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your siblings NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner’s siblings NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your extended family NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner's extended family NA 1 2 3 4 5 Lesbian, bisexual, andlor gay friends NA 1 2 3 4 5 Heterosexual friends NA 1 2 3 4 5 Lesbian I bisexual I gay community NA 1 2 3 4 5 Local community, neighbors M 1 2 3 4 5 Acquaintances through church or religious groups NA 1 2 3 4 5 Coworkers NA 1 2 3 4 5 Business associates NA 1 2 3 4 5 Schoolmates NA 1 2 3 4 5 Professors 1 teachers at school NA 1 2 3 4 5 Malth care workers (doctors, nurses, therapists, etc) 53 APPENDIX A Perceived Prejudice: Indicate on a scale from 1 to 5 how much prejudice, or anti-homosexual sentiment. you perceive in each of the following areas, using this rating scale: 1 - no prejudice 2 - low degree of prejudice 3 - some degree of prejudice 4 - moderate degree of prejudice 5- very him degreeof prejudice NA - not applicable, no contact with this group (circleonenurrberor'M'foreachitem) NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your children NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner's children NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your parents M 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner’s parents NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your siblings NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner’s sblings NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your extended family NA 1 2 3 4 5 Your partner's extended family NA 1 2 3 4 5 Lesbian, bisexual, and/or gay friends NA 1 2 3 4 5 Heterosexual friends M 1 2 3 4 5 Lesbian / bisexual / gay community M 1 2 3 4 5 Local community. neighbors M 1 2 3 4 5 Acquaintances through church or religious groups M 1 2 3 4 5 Coworkers M 1 2 3 4 5 Business associates M 1 2 3 4 5 Schoolmates M 1 2 3 4 5 Professors lteachers at school M 1 2 3 4 5 Health care workers (doctors, nurses, therapists, etc) 54 APPENDIX A Relationship information: Are you currently in a romantic I sexual relationship? No Yes If not, answer the following items on the basis of your most recent significant relationship. Indicate how long ago this relationship ended: _years months (if you’ve never been in a romantic / sexual relationship, check here and skip this page.) How many children does your partner have? if more than zero, number under 18 years of age if more than zero, number that live in your household How long have you and your partner been a couple? years months is your partner female or male ? Which best describes your agreement about sexual exclusivity with your partner? (check one) conpietely monogamous / exclusive mostly monogamous open relationship no specific agreement at this tlrne lndlcateonascalefrom1toStnwsatisfiedyouarewflheachofthefolbwingareasofyomrelaflonship using this rating scale: 1 - very dissatisfied 2 - somewhat dissatisfied 3 - mostly satisfied 4 - satisfied 5 - very satisfied (circle one number for each item) Frequency of sexual contact Quality of sexual contact 1 2 3 4 5 Emotional intimacy with partner 1 2 3 4 5 Social intlrnacy with partner 1 2 3 4 5 Intellectual intimacy with partner 1 2 3 4 5 Recreational intimacy with partner 1 2 3 4 5 Sexual intimacy with partner 1 2 3 4 5 Level of affection expressed by partner toward you 1 2 3 4 5 Level of affection expressed by you toward partner 1 2 3 4 5 Amount of tlrne spent with partner 1 2 3 4 5 Living situation 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 55 APPENDIX A Self-description: Use each of the following personality characteristics to describe yourself. That is, indicate on a scale from 1 to 7 how true of you each of these characteristics is, using this rating scale: dd-l-h-L—b-A-‘dddd-‘d—A—L—A—‘d—‘d—l-fi—ld—A—‘dd—l MMMNNNMNNNNNMNMNNNNNNMNNNNMMNN (#030030303000)QQQQQGQQQQQQQUQQQQQODQ 1 - Never or airnost never true 2 - Usually not true 3 - Sometimes but infrequently true 4 - Occasionally true 5 - Often true 6 - Usually true 7 - Always or almost always tme 4 5 7 ##&bbhbhbbbb55#&hhbb#bbbb~hbbb urcntnmmmtncncnmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmm diorama:mmmmarmmmmmmmmmmommmmmmmmm VNVVVVVVVVVNNNVVVVV‘JVVV‘IVVV‘JV (circle one number for each item) Defend my own beliefs Affectionate Conscientious independent Sympathetic Moody Assertive Sensitive to needs of others Reliable Strong personality Understanding Jealous Forceful Compassionate Truthful Have leadership abilities Eager to sooth hurt feelings Secretive Willing to take risks Warm Adaptable Dominant Tender Conceited Willing to take a stand Love children Tactfui Aggressive Gentle Conventional 56 APPENDIX A in your own words, describe some strategies you find helpful in dealing with prejudice, and in what kinds of situations you might use particular strategies. Feel free to use examples from your own experience—What strategies worked or might work? What strategies didn’t work or might not work? APPENDIX B APPENDIX B POSTER SOLICITATION VOLUNTEERS NEEDED Lesbian and bisexual women needed for questionnaire study on dealing with prejudice Please take a questionnaire for yourself, and I or one for a friend Completely anonymous— please don’t write your name on questionnaire Thank you for your help! This study is sponsored by Michigan State University For more information, contact Katari Brown (616) 781 -9353 57 APPENDIX C APPENDIX C m“. en. mu. N“. E mptmpmpepmpwpppopmmnmmemNP 8. .- .88. -88 .2... 8.8 8.. 9.8 2.... mm... 8.. .3 8.. 8.. Re 8.. , 09,80 mud mad mud and 05d mud 00.0 05.0 eed emd emd med med ~0d 50.0 de 00.0 05.0 emd Omd emd de 0nd wed emd 00d mod mod mmd mmd 5d 3.0 Ned and mmd mmd and Ned wed Pmd med 8.0 end Ned 0~d mmd and de end Gd emd Omd mmd eod 00.0 50.0 mmd Gd mmd Ed and nmd mNd and Ed end smd mmd cod emd nod emd Rd 00.0 00.0 wed Ped 0nd end end Ned 5.0 55.0 emd mad mud ewd eed mud msd Egg nmd mod 3.0 50.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 nmd Fed 0~d med wed 0nd Ed eed Fed 50d emd mnd 8d mud mmd e0d Kd N0d mmd -d eed pmd 0~d mmd Ned and ~0d 00d ~0d e0d med 50.0 50.0 00.0 0nd de nmd mmd _.ed med Ned and 50.0 00.0 and 8.0 00.0 mud 00d and 0~d 0ed and Fed mmd omd ped mmd med 3.0 00.0 ~0d mmd e~d Fwd med and and med Fed emd 2d med 5.0 e0d emd emd -d mmd med pmd mmd 00.0 5.0 mmd 00d Kd 00d Fed nmd Ned wed eed omd 0ed Ned e0d de mmd end 0m.o umd wmd mmd oed med med ~0d omd and and m~d e~d mpd omd 0~d Fwd e~d med 0.3.033... m. 6.930%) a. 0.30.0593...) m @8350} a 0.0530} c 3326 £88353} .0. $98} A. main?) 0. gang} m. 3.28} v. 3518853} m. 8.5.5.3... ~. £1888...) .. 5.83:0... o. 00.0 9.0 2.0 5.0 0.0 0N0 -.0 0.0 0m.0 3.8.3.8.} m 00.0 {.0 00.0 00.0 med 0m.0 00.0 $0 ginseng} 0 e00 00.0 0e.0 med and end 3.0 $985!} \. 00.0 and 0e.0 8.0 e00 3.0 Eng} 0 0e.0 00.0 end 50.0 00.0 guinea} m 3.0 050 00.0 96 £1. e E0 med e~.0 E} m ~00 ~00 Stream-Ex; N ~00 Samoa} — “fame sagassngasvaog gage x3e: some—9:00 3.3.0 was Es: 835:0 "a wish 58 59 APPENDIX C me e“. m“. Nu. E m. 8...- «001000.000 00.0 8; 9d di pod 8.0 00.0 5.0. do._. mpd- 8.0 g ~.0.0.v.0.~...0. 0 0 A 0 0 v 0 ~. 00.0 ~00 :0 0~.0 0~.0 00.0 00 ~00 0~0 .00 90 ~00 00.0 00.0 0.0 3.0 00.0 ~00 0.05.03... 00 -0 0.0 .~.0 0.0 ~..0 ~..0 0.0 ~..0 00.0 00.0 00.0 0.0 .00 0.0 8.0 8.0 0.0 0.0. 6.0035033 a .~.0 00.0 0~.0 .00 00.0 0~.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 0.0 0~.0 00.0 ~00 3.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 0~.0- 00.05.80.303 0“. 00.0 8.0 00.0 ~00 3.0 00.0 000 ~..0 80 0.0 00.0 00.0 3.0 00.0 0.0 .00 00.0- 8.0. 00025503 m 00.0 ~00 0.0 00.0 .00 00.0 .00. ~00 ~00. 00.0 0.0 -.0. ~00. 2.0 .00. 00.0. 00.0 00.0 305303 .0 .00 9.0 ~..0 00.0 00.0 .~.0 .00 0~.0 Q0 0~.0 0~0 3.0 00.0 00.0 2.0 ~..0 0~.0 0.9 meanness! 0. 0. 00.0 00.0 00.0 .00 0~.0 00.0 0~.0 0.0 00.0 0~.0 00.0 0~.0 00.0 0~.0 0~.0 00.0 .00 8.98 a A. .00 00.0 00.0 0.0 00.0 0~.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 -0 2.0 9.0 .00 00.0 ~..0 ~00 3.3930. 00.0 2.0 -.0 00.0 .00 0.0 0.0 0~.0 -0 0.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 -.0 .~.0 seasonal... 0. 0. 8.0 0~.0 ~00 8.0 ...0 0.0 00.0 00.0 0~.0 ~00 ~00 0~.0 0.0 00.0- 00.28 a v. 0~.0 00.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0~.0 0~.0 0.0 00.0 ~00 00.0 00.0 0.0 gen—0 0. 0. 00.0 00.0 0.0 0.0 0~.0 ~00 0~.0 ~00 0~.0 00.0 0.0 3.0. 03.5.35. -0 00:0 8.0 0~.0 0~.0 0~.0 0~.0 .00 0~.0 00.0 0.0 810188.06. 0. .. #0 E0 .00 ...0 00.0 00.0 .00 ~00 0.0 0.0. €50.39. 0.0. R0 ...0 ~..0 .00- 0.0 00.0 0.0 3.0 000. 0.5.0339. b 0 0.0 00.0 ~00 .00 0.0 -0 0.0 8.0 Engage 0 $0 000 -.0 0~.0 0~.0 .80 00.0. 6500.05! a A 00.0 0~.0 00.0 00.0 0.0- .~.0. anaemia! 0. 0 0~.0 00.0 00.0 0.0 ~00 353.0830 00.0 00.0 0~.0. 0.0- 5:030 00.0 00.0. ~00. nil 0. 0 ~00 ~00 5300.33 3 ~ ~..0 6000:.» a . l "$508 0. 80.32 :30 0800285.. ”0.9. 80%;. 5.302 sewn—ebony 0390.0 0:0 :8: 3:03on 00385.0 "N 200,—. 60 m“. N... E : op m 0 m 0 m e m N — 00.0 Ned med 00.. 00.0 00.. ”0:250:00 030$ APPENDIX C de de 0ed Omd mNd 0~.0 mmd mmd Fed mNd 0ed NNd mmd 00d mmd 0md 00.0 00.0 mmd Ned 0~.0 Ned mNd med mmd emd 00d e0d mod mmd m0d 00.0 00.0 ”0:000. :00 . 60... EB. Fwd pmd N~d m..d mpd N..d mpd mpd NNd 00d mmd pmd eed 0~.0 mNd eNd Pmd Pmd mmd eNd mmd med mmd 00d de oed de 0~.0 opd mmd N0d 0md Ned med med mmd mNd 0md de med Fwd med med mNd oed Fed end mmd oed oed eed med 00d oed mmd med dmd end mmd 00d med pmd 00d Ned mmd e0d 2 703005000058 0000005000.. n“. 6.0.5 8.000005% .00 xmm we .0 2... $0988.00. 60505 .0 8:03.00032 .. 00500030000000: 0. 88:8 .0300 00 500000: 0 0000000 .0300 00 5:000 m 60.05:. .00 .00 m 000300 >0 00.000000 0 005000 >0 00.000000 0 30550. $850.6 e .6082... 88:005.: 0 60.000. .0383... N 60.0.00. .0600 . 500%06 5 02:30:00 830 00030-2000 EB. “9:0: 39.0323. 0.0502 coma—ebeu Stem use So: :emtawmmam inane—ey— nm 030,—. 61 APPENDIX C Table 4: Outness/ Perceived Prejudice Correlations by Gender Role Group statistical 95% confidence interval Group Correlation Value N significance lower bound upper bound Sample r op-aggregate - O . 3 7 344 p<.05 -O.42 -O. 3 2 Androgynous r op-aggregate - O . 3 3 1 36 p<.05 -0.41 -0. 2 5 Masculine r op-aggregate - 0 . 4 6 91 p<.05 -O. 54 -0. 38 Feminine r op-aggregate - 0 . 5 8 43 p<.05 -0.68 -O.48 Undifferentiated r op-aggregate - O . O 8 3 5 NS -0. 2 5 0.09 Sample r op-ownfamiiy - O . 4 6 333 p<.05 -0. SO -O.42 Androgynous r op-ownfamily - O . 3 6 1 31 p<.05 -0.44 -0.28 Masculine r op-ownfamiiy - 0 . 6 0 90 p<.OS -O. 67 -0. 5 3 Feminine r op-ownfamiiy -O . 5 5 42 p<.05 -0.66 -O.44 Undifferentiated r op-ownfamr’ly - O . 4 0 3 3 p<.OS -0. S S -0. 2 5 Sample r op-partnerfam - O . 4 4 216 p<.OS -0.49 -0. 39 Androgynous r op-partnerfam — 0 . 2 8 93 p<.05 -O. 38 -O.1 8 Masculine r op-partnerfam — 0 . S 6 58 p<.05 -O.65 -0.47 Feminine r op-partnerfam - O . S 2 27 p<.OS -O.66 -0.38 Undifferentiated r op-partnerfam - O . 5 4 1 3 p<.OS -O. 74 -0. 34 Sample r op-pubiic -O . 3 S 342 p<.05 -0.40 -0.30 Androgynous r op—pubiic - O . 2 9 1 32 p<.05 -0. 3 7 -O. 2 1 Masculine r op-pubiic -0 . 3 8 87 p<.OS -O.47 -0.29 Feminine r op-public -O. 7 1 43 p<.05 -O.79 ~0.63 Undifferentiated r op-public - 0 . O4 35 NS -O.21 0.13 r op-aggregate: Correlation of average outness ratings with average perceived prejudice ratings r op-ownfamr'iy: Correlation between the average score of items from factors Outness with Own Family and Prejudice in Own Family r op-partnerfam: Correlation between the average score of items from factors Outness with Partner’s Family and Prejudice in Partner’s Family r op-pubir‘c: Correlation between the average score of items from factors Outness in Public and Prejudice in Public 62 APPENDIX C 0:20 :83an0 BEE: :8: .832 .093: 5.0 - - - 00.m 33:53:85; - m0.0 - - 00.m o:_:_Eo....:oz - - - - 05m 2:382:02 - - - 5.0 ~m.m m§>9:0:<-32 - - 5.0 5.0 0 fin 33:53:35 - mod 5.0 0~.m o:_:_Eou - - 05m 3:383: - 000 32523:" o33> madam :3: 35. 8:85:03 00:34.. 3232728535 $55 ”was: 9.5.0 :83an0 REE: .Eo... .332 0935‘ 5.0 - - - mm m 033353255002 - - - - mm m o:_:_Eou_-:oz - - - - mm m «53832-52 - - - 8.0 an 3232.05-82 - 0 30 5.0 5.0 tam 33:58:65 - - m0.0 5.m 32:50“. .. 0 P .0 0~.m 2:383: - mm.m «30:50:92 m53> 039.0 :30: 0:20 :OmtSEoo L505 .Eou .omaFdQfiE‘ 0 p .0 .. - - ta 33:53:35-52 .. no.0 - - ~06 m:.:_Eo.._-:oz - - - - 55 o:__:om32-:oz - - - 8.0 gm “Scxmocuc<-coz - - 0 p .0 0 p .0 N 3m 33:53:35 - no.0 no.0 0 p .m 055:8”. .. - 005 3:33: - 5.m 26:30:24. 33> 05mm :35 11.26. 088533 3.33 .32 s .553"— mtufiuamlacoataficu 385.5 8 «33,—. .26. 8:85:03. vo_.3-~ .32 0 has—am zip—833500 $2.50 "0 033,—. 0:20 :omtmanu ED :5... duo? .0832. 5.0 - - - 5.m vouazcocoticnéoz - no.0 - - 00.m o:_:_Eod-:oz - - - - vmd 3:332:02 - - - 5o 83 macaenczoz - - 5 0 5.0 0m.m 33:53:55 - 0P 0 5.0 mm.m 855:8”. - - 004” 3:33: - 006 msocaaocvc< o23> $15.3 can: .26. 088:_&W8__3-~ .32 : eaglesgfiou 385:0 um 23:. 63 APPENDIX C 0:90 50.50.50 0:05 .500 .0002 .6055. $0 83:50:35-52 - 00.0 .. - aim 055500-52 - - - - mm.~ 05.:0002-52 - - - - mmd 0:05605:<-52 - - - - 00.N 53:53:55 - 5.0 5.0 0~.N 055500 - - 00d 0530002 - 0v.~ 00056055. 0:3> 0:000 :00: .05. 00:35:90 00.30-N :8. 0 0.3038358 8352.. a: 2.5 050 5050.50 0:05 .500 .0002 .0850. 00.0 - n p .m 003::20505-52 - - - - mud 055500-52 - - - - 00.N 053032-52 - - - mod 80 888905-82 - - o F .0 5.0 N P .m 0035:0352: - - - 00.~ 055500 - - m0.~ 0530032 - m0.~ 0556055 0:3> 0:000 :00: .05. 00:00am 00.3fm :00: .0 >25"— :30'0000:00500 00:030...— 5~ 030,—. 0:80 505050 Eb .50“? .000: .005: 0:0 - - - .03. 00:05:53 00:20 0:80 50.3050 NNNcriNNdori MNmmCDv-MW mva—QN‘DI— 0:3> c a 0 5 5350.05.05-52 055500-52 053032-52 055695352 53:53:35 055500 0530002 05560550. 0~.0 :00. .0 5:30 0.005301050030600 00:00.3..— "2 030k E5 .500 .0002 605:4. Nv.~ :03520505-52 - - .. - Saw 055500-52 - - - - 0¢.~ 05.:0002-52 - - - - 00d 0:0560550-52 - - 0 _ .0 - Rio. 53:55:55 - - - and 055500 - - 0m.~ 0530002 - mm.~ 05569.5( 0:3) 0:93 :00: .0>0_ 00:00:36.0 00__3- :00. .0 80000021000530.000 00:00.3...— 0 030,—. APPENDIX C 0:80 5050500 0:80 5050500 .505 .50“. .802 .0205. 5.25 .500 .80: .0205 - mv.m 00055050552502 09m 00003550055502 - - - - 02m 055500-52 - - - - 00.0.” 055500-52 - - - - :V.m 05.:0002-52 - - - - 00.m 05.30002-52 - - - - o¢.m 05560550502 - - - - and 0:0:60.5:.<-:02 - - - - mm.m 00005050055 - - - - mm.m 080.50.000.05 - - - 0~.m 055500 - - - mm.m 055500 - - on.m 05.:0002 - - mm.m 05.:0002 - 9am 0556055. - _. Em 0:05605:< 5000:8050 0:005:90 0:0 0:_0> Magd- 300555: 0:005:90 0:0 020) 0: m0 :002 :002 .20. 8:80:00 02.2-0 .96. 8:850: 3.2.0 1 :00. s 00050500900080.5500 50000.0: "3 030,—. :00. 3 530000000 53001000050500 50020—00 “my 0300. 0:80 505050 0:80 5050500 005:: .500 .0002 .6805. 005:: .500 .0002 68—05.. - - - - mm.m 00055800055502 - - - - om.m 00005050553502 - - - - 064m 055500-52 - - - - mud 055500-52 - - mod - moé 05.:0002-52 - - - - mad 05 :0002-52 - - - o 20 om.m 0556055502 - - - - Mn...” 05:» 550-52 - - - - ~05 0000.50.03.05 - - - - 09m 0303:8055: - o P .o - C 2: 055500 - - - 80 055500 - mod 50 0550002 - - :5 0530002 - 00.0 0556055. - 000 05568.5( 80> al.093- 0082050 28:20.0 9: 30> M0045 :002 :002 .05. 00:00:5E0 :0._0u- .05. 00:85:90 :0__0u- :00. .0 005000000 50505003100500 50000.0: "3 030... $00. 00020000200 00000005000000.5800 .00—000.0: “my 030,—. APPENDD( D APPENDIX D Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated Gender Role Group Figure 1: Mean Aggregate Outness by Group M 5.00e e a 4.00a n Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated 3.00 * 2.00 ’ 1.004 0 u t n e s s 0.00 4 Gender Role Group Figure 2: Mean Aggregate Prejudice by Group 65 66 APPENDIX D Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated Gender Role Group Figure 3: Mean Outness in Own Family by Group M 5.00‘ ' Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated e a 4.00 * Gender Role Group Figure 4: Mean Prejudice in Own Family by Group 67 APPENDIX D Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated Gender Role Group Figure 5: Mean Outness in Partner’ 5 Family by Group Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated Gender Role Group Figure 6: Mean Prejudice in Partner’ 5 Family by Group 68 APPENDIX D M 5.00 ’ e Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated a 4.00 a n 3.00 a O u .i t 2.00 n €1.00! s s 0.00 '- Gender Role Group Figure 7: Mean Outness in Public by Group e Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated Gender Role Group Figure 8: Mean Prejudice in Public by Group 69 APPENDIX D Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated Gender Role Group Figure 9: Mean Outness in Les-bi-gay Group e Androgynous Masculine Feminine Undifferentiated a 4'oo_i ............................. , ......................................................................................... n Gender Role Group Figure 10: Mean Prejudice in Les-bi-gay Group BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Alain, M. 8: Lussier, Y. (1988). Sex-role attitudes and divorce experience. Journal of Social Psychology, 128 (2), 143—152. l Anderson, T. & Leitner, L. (1991). The relationship between the r Defense Mechanisms Inventory and reported symptomatology in college females. Personality and Individual Diflerences, 12 (9), 967-969. L Antill, J. 6: Cunningham, J. (1979). Self—esteem as a function of masculinity in both sexes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47 (4), 783-785. Babledelis, G. (197 8). Sex-role concepts and flexibility on measures of thinking, feeling, and behaving. Psychological Reports, 42 (1), 99-105. Barbarin, O. (1983). Coping with ecological transitions by Black families: A psychosocial model. Journal of Community Psychology, 11 (4), 308-322. Bern, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42 (2), 155-162. Bem, S. L. (1979). Theory and measurement of androgyny: a reply to the Pedhazur-Tetenbaum and Locksley-Colten critiques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (6), 1047-1054. Bern, S. L. 8: Lewis, S. (1975). Sex role adaptability: One consequence of psychological androgyny. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31 (4), 634-643. Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) Manual (1981) copyright by Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., Distributed by Mind Garden, Palo Alto, CA Blumstein, P. 8: Schwartz, P. (1983). American Couples: Money, Work, Sex. New York: William Morrow. 70 71 Borders, L. & Pong, M. (1984). Sex-role orientation research: Review and implications for counselor education. Counselor Education and Supervision, 24 (1), 58-69. Brown, LS. (1986). Confronting internalized oppression in sex therapy with lesbians. Journal of Homosexuality, 12 (3-4), 99-107. Clunis, D.M. 8: Green, GD. (1993). Lesbian Couples: Creating Healthy Couples for the ’905. Seattle: Seal Press. Dancey, CF. (1992). The relationship of instrumentality and expressivity to sexual orientation in women. Journal of Homosexuality, 23 (4), 71-82. Duffy, S.M. (1985—96). Satisfaction and commitment in homosexual and heterosexual relationships. Journal of Homosexualtiy, 12 (2), 1-23. Dion, K.L., Dion, K.I(. & Pak, A.W. (1992). Personality-based hardiness as a buffer for discrimination-related stress in members of Toronto’s Chinese community. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 24 (4), 517-536. Donelson, E. (1995). Gender Related Views of Self and Others. Unpublished article. Eliason, M., Donelan, C. & Randall, C. (1992). lesbian stereotypes. Health Care for Women International, 13, 131-144. Ellis, ].B. & Range, L.M. (1988). Femininity and reasons for living. Educational and Psychological Research, 8 (1), 19-24. Glazebrook, CK & Munjas, BA. (1986). Sex roles and depression. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 24 (12), 9-12. Heilbrun, AB. (1981). Gender differences on the functional linkage between androgyny, social cognition, and competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41 (6), 1106-1118. Heilbrun, AB. 8: Han, Y.L. (1986). Sex differences in the adaptive value of androgyny. Psychological Reports, 59 (3), 1023-1-26. Heilbrun, A.B. & Mulqueen, C.M . (1987). The second androgyny: A proposed revision in adaptive priorities for college women. Sex Roles, 17 (3- 4), 187-207. 72 Heilbrun, A.B. & Pitman, D. (1979). Testing some basic assumptions about psychological androgyny. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 135 (2), 175— 188. Heilbrun, A.B. & Schwartz, H.L. (1982). Sex-gender differences in level of androgyny. Sex Roles, 8 (2), 201-214. Herek, GM. (1993). Documenting prejudice against lesbians and gay men on campus: The Yale Sexual Orientation Study. Journal of Homosexuality, 25 (4), 15-30. Ho, R. (1981). Sex, sex-role typing, and children’s problem-solving behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 115 (2), 219-226. Hurlbert, DP. 8: Apt, C. (1993). Female sexuality: A comparative study between women in homosexual and heterosexual relationships. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 19 (4), 315—327. Iasenza, S. (1991),. The relations among selected aspects of sexual orientation and sexual functioning in females. Dissertation, New York University, Ph.D. in School of Education, Health, Nursing, and Arts Professions. Jones, E.E., Farina, A., Hastorf, A.H., Markus, H., Miller, D.T., Scott, R.A., French, R. de S. (1984). Social Stigma: The Psychology of Marked Relationships. New York: Freeman. Katchadourian, HA. (1989). Fundamentals of Human Sexuality (5th ed.). Orlando: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Kurdek, L. A. (1991). Correlates of relationship satisfaction in cohabiting gay and lesbian couples: Integration of contextual, investment, and problem-solving models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6 910-922. Kimlicka, T., Cross, H. & Tamai, I. (1983). A comparison of androgynous, feminine, masculine, and undifferentiated women on self- esteem, body satisfaction, and sexual satisfaction. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 7 (3), 291-294. Kivlighan, D.M., Clements, L., Blake, C. & Arnzen, A. (1993). Counselor sex role orientation, flexibility, and working alliance formation. Journal of Counseling and Development, 72 (1), 95-100. 73 Kleinke, C.L. & Hinrichs, CA. (1983). College adjustment problems and attitudes toward drinking reported by feminine, androgynous, and masculine college women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 7 (3), 291-294. Krausz, M., Kedem, P., Ta], 2. & Amir, Y. (1992). Sex-role orientation and work adaption of male nurses. Research in Nursing and Health, 15 (5), 391 -398. Kurdek, LA. (1986). Early development of relationship quality in heterosexual married, heterosexual cohabiting, gay, and lesbian couples. Developmental Psychology, 22 (3), 305-309. Lee, AG. and Schreurer, V.L. (1983). Psychological androgyny and aspects of self-image in women and men. Sex Roles, 9 (3), 289-306. Lazerson, IS. (1981). Psychological androgyny, sex stereotyping, and mental health. (Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, 1981) Leserman, J., DiSantostefano, R., Perkins, DD. 8: Evans, BL. (1994). Gay identification and psychological health in HIV-positive and HIV - negative gay men. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24 (24), 2193-2208. Lobel, T.E., Gur, S. & Yerushalrni, H. (1989). Cheating behavior of sex- type and androgynous children in sex-stereotyped and no-sex-stereotyped tasks. Journal of Research in Personality, 23 , 302-312. Locksley, A. fit Colten, M.E. (1979). Psyhcological androgyny: A case of mistaken identity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 6, 1017- 1031. Loulan, J. (1988). Research on the sex practices of 1566 lesbians and the clinical applications. Women and Therapy, 7 (2-3), 221-234. Lykes, MB. (1983). Discrimination and coping in the lives of black women: Analyses of oral history data. Journal of Social Issues, 39 (3), 79-100. Marecek, I. (1979). Social change, positive mental health, and psychological androgyny. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 3 (3), 241-247. Marsh, H.W. 8t Byrne, BM. (1991). Differentiated additive model: Relations between masculinity, femininity, and multiple dimensions of self- concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (5), 811-828. 74 Mihalik, GJ. (1991). From anthropology: Homosexuality, stigma, and biocultural evolution. Special section: Stigma and homosexuality. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Psychotherapy, 1 (4), 15-29. Nichols, M. (1982). The treatment of inhibited sexual desire disorder (ISD) in lesbian couples. Women and Therapy, 1 (4), 49-66. Oldham, S. (1982). Sex role identity of female homosexuals. Journal of Homosexuality, 8 (1), 41-46. Orlofsky, J.L. 81: Windle, MT (1978). Sex-role orientation, behavioral adaptability and personal adjustment. Sex Roles, 4 (6), 801-811. Page, S. St Yee, M. (1985). Conception of male and female homosexual stereotypes among university undergraduates. Journal of Homosexuality, 12 (1), 109-118. Pehazur, E]. 8: Tetenbaum, TJ. (1979). Bem sex role inventory: A theoretical and methodological critique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (6), 996-1016. Peters, D.K. & Cantrell, PJ. (1991). Factors distinguishing samples of lesbian and heterosexual women. Journal of Homosexuality, 21 (4), 1-15. Phinney, ].S. 8: Chavira, V. (1995). Parental ethnic socialization and adolescent coping with problems related to ethnicity. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 5 (1), 31-53. Pond, J.H. (1984). Psychological androgyny and adjustment: An interpersonal conceptualization of the adaptive consequences of sex-role orientation (Doctoral dissertation, University of Saskatchewan, 1984). Prager, K]. 8: Bailey, ].M. (1985). Androgyny, ego development, and psycvhosocial crisis resolution. Sex Roles, 13 (9-10), 525-536. Pyke, SW. (1985). Androgyny: An integration. special issue: Sex roles and sex differences and androgyny. International Journal of Women ’5 Studies, 8 (5), 529-539. Safir, M.P., Peres, Y., Lichtenstein, M., Hoch, Z., 81: Shepher, J. (1982). Psychological androgyny and sexual adequacy. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 8 (3), 228-240. Schreurs, KM.G. (1993). Sexuality in lesbian couples: The importance of gender. Annual Review of Sex Research, 4, 49—66. 75 Spence, ].T. & Hehnreich, KL. (1981). Androgyny versus gender schema: A comment on Bem’s gender schema theory. Psychological Review, 88, (4), 365-368. Spence, J.T., Helmreich, KL. 8: Stapp (1975). Ratings of self and peers on sex role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity and feminity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, (1), 29-39. Stites, MC. (1983). Mental health services for gay students: Gay- straight Rap. Journal of American College Health, 32 (2), 86-87. Tidball, K. (1990). Application of coping strategies developed by older deaf adults to the aging process. American Annals of the Deaf, 135 (1), 33-40. Waterman, A.S. & Whitbourne, S.I(. (1982). Androgyny and psychosocial development among college students and adults. Journal of Personality, 50 (2), 121-133. Wiggins, IS. 5: Holzmuller, A. (1981). Further evidence on androgyny and interpersonal flexibility. Journal of Research on Personality, 15 (1), 67-80. "illilllllllillllllllllli